68 sajcd • vol 59 • december 2012 cpd december 2012 1. true (a) or false (b): research has shown that very low birth weight has a severe impact on hearing. 2. true (a) or false (b): in developed countries like the usa hiv is included as a high risk factor for hearing loss. 3. true (a) or false (b): a study on neonates with very low birth weight showed that exposure to ototoxic medication was the most commonly occurring risk factor for hearing loss. 4. true (a) or false (b): context-specific risk factors are relevant for effective targeted hearing screening protocols. 5. true (a) or false (b): a study on neonates with very low birth weight revealed a statistically significant relationship between risk factors and distortion product otoacoustic emissions (dpoae) screening results. 6. true (a) or false (b): the relationship between language, mathematics and numeracy is well understood. 7. true (a) or false (b): internalisation refers to learning that occurs on a social plane. 8. true (a) or false (b): cognitive academic language proficiency (calp) typically precedes the development of basic interactive communicative skills (bics). 9. true (a) or false (b): research revealed that teacher expectations of learners’ achievement in low-income communities are typically low. 10. true (a) or false (b): owing to their cost-effectiveness, cochlear implants are used in high volumes worldwide. 11. true (a) or false (b): in the uk, individuals and families carry some direct costs for cochlear implant services. 12. true (a) or false (b): the first multi-channel cochlear implantation in south africa took place in 1989. 13. true (a) or false (b): the total cost for an adult cochlear implantation in south africa is less than for a child. 14. true (a) or false (b): a similar number of vowels are used in most spoken languages. 15. true (a) or false (b): a swahili screening test for speech in neurological disorders was developed for sole use by speech therapists. 16. true (a) or false (b): the swahili speech screening test included a swallowing assessment. 17. true (a) or false (b): the speech loudness of patients with parkinson’s disease and the control group differed significantly with the swahili speech screening test. 18. true (a) or false (b): stress and intonation patterns have an impact on the translation of english test items into isizulu. 19. true (a) or false (b): male participants often used the word ‘please’ during the translation of western aphasia battery (wab) test items. 20. true (a) or false (b): spatial release of masking (srm) refers to the improvement in speech perception as a result of spatial separation of speech and noise when listening with both ears. cpd questionnaires must be completed online via www.cpdjournals.co.za. after submission you can check the answers and print your certificate. the south african journal of communication disorders sajcd we are pleased to announce that the number of ceus per test has been increased to 5. 3 myrtle l. aron claire penn, ph.d (witwatersrand) head, department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg professor myrtle lily aron retired this year after over thirty six years service to the profession of speech pathology and audiology in south africa. these four decades have been some of the most exciting and influential in the history of south africa and the development and growth of the profession during this time reflects the qualities, leadership and insight of a person particularly sensitive to the changing times and the needs of the country and its people. she was born in 1929 on the east rand, during the depression, and though she rarely makes reference to these difficult times, it is certain that her early childhood made her particularly sensitive to the needs of less privileged sectors of the population. she attended the university of the witwatersrand as a student in the sub-department of logopedics and its speech voice and hearing clinic under professor ρ de v pienaar, obtaining her ba logopedics in 1953. she continued to work in this department as an assistant supervisor until 1954 when she left for a year to work as a clinical speech therapist in the department of psychiatry at the university of toronto, canada. she returned to south africa and in 1956 embarked on her research for her master's degree which involved an investigation of the nature and incidence of stuttering among a bantu group of school-going children. this research, culminating in the award of a master of arts cum laude in 1959, was a seminal work in the area of stuttering and its measurement. still frequently cited in current stuttering tests, its findings as well as its rigorous attention to methodological issues placed south africa on the map in the field of stuttering. her research forj her doctoral degree which was awarded in 1964, was on the effects of the combination of trifluoperazine and amyloba'rbitcme on adult stutterers. while her research and teaching activities moved to other areas after this degree, her keen interest in the area of stuttering remained and was nowhere more evident than in the organization and planning of the department's jubilee international stuttering conference held at the university of the witwatersrand in 1986. while undertaking her research, she was employed in the subdepartment as a clinician and assistant supervisor (19561958) and then as a clinical lecturer and tutor (1959-1964). in 1964 she was awarded an ernest oppenheimer memorial trust award and a fullbright hayes travel grant in order to study in the field of audiology in the united states. she spent a year at the university of pittsburg and visiting training institutions in the united states and the united kingdom. on her return to south africa, she developed new courses in audiology and her influence in this aspect in the profession condie suid-afrikaansc tydskrif \ir kommitiiikitsirafirykiiiys, vol. 37, 1990 tinued to grow and develop. she was very active in the needs of the deaf in south africa and has written memory ndi#oη early intervention for the infant and young hearing impaired. she was a member of the audiological technical committee and steering committee on acoustics and noise abatement for the south african bureau of standards, and a fellow of the south african acoustics institute. her inaugural lecture, delivered in 1974, was entitled communication for the hearing impaired some plain talk, and presented some hard-hitting facts about the nature of deaf education in the country and some clear directions for its improvement. a significant step in her career and acknowledgement of her abilities came when the sub-department gained full status in july 1971 and became the department of speech pathology and audiology. shortly thereafter she was appointed as full professor and head of the department and director of the speech and hearing clinic, a post which she held until her retirement earlier this year. this acknowledgement of the department's independent status more or less coincided with the move of the department, previously housed in the library basement and then in yale cottage, to its present location the social sciences block. just as she was instrumental in creating the department spiritually, it is true to say that her role in the physical development of the new department was pivotal. she was involved in each step of its design and its construction and it has served as an impressive model for later student training facilities in this country. besides the many hundreds of undergraduate students who have passed through her hands and who will remember her lectures in a wide range of topics, she has been involved wit! the supervision of many postgraduate students. her keen inte rests in research methodology, together with her experience and her insights, both academic and emotional, were highl} valued by her postgraduate students. her involvement in the university and in broader issue: related to the community has been tireless. she stood on £ number of important university committees, including the boards of the faculties of arts and medicine. she resisted fre quent attempts by the university to change the faculty ο affiliation, and its close and lasting relationship to the arts faculty has been the hallmark of the course at wits and ofter the envy of other departments both locally and inter nationally. this close link with the humanities and with th« disciplines of psychology and linguistics particularly is a reflec tion of her attitude to life and particularly to rehabilitatior £ sash a 199c r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 claire penn other university committees on which she served during her career were the higher degreescommittee, the human ethics research committee, the executive committee of convocation, and the senate publications committee, as well as several scholarship awards committees. professor aron's long involvement in community issues has perhaps been nowhere more evident than in her efforts through the south african speech-language-hearing association to establish the two-year diploma course for speech and hearing therapy community workers. this course, which started at wits university in 1984, was the first of its kind in the world. its aim was to train persons who could identify and manage the communication problems of the broader community in south africa particularly in the rural'areas. this has proven to be very successful and at least 46 diploma graduates are now working in the field and providing a very important service to populations previously having no help. her own research and energy in this field has done a lot to establish the groundwork for this course and has provided directions for course content, training and employment opportunities. this has included field trips to gazankulu and the transkei where she has examined the need for essential services set up networks and set up long-term research plans. she is also a founder member of ruract (rural disability action group) and a member of the working group to consider an institute of urban primary health care based on the alexandra health centre. she was also requested to act as chairman of a coordinating committee of senior persons from the professions of occupational therapy, physiotherapy and speech therapy and audiology to explore and establish community rehabilitation education and training of rehabilitation workers in south africa. her efforts in this regard have been publicized internationally and have been presented at the international association of logopedics and phoniatrics congresses in tokyo (1986) and in czechoslovakia (1989). the course has been a model, not only for other professions but also for workers in other countries. it was also through her efforts that the unit for languageand hearing-impaired children was established at the university. now housed at the transvaal memorial institute this facility provides facilities for a number of preschool children who require a specially integrated approach to rehabilitation. for a long time the only such unit in the transvaal, it has provided an exceptional training and research facility for students and become a nationally recognized facility addressing a desperate shortage in this country. professor aron also instituted a long-term research programme on the development and maintenance of a computerized information retrieval programme of case data on communication disorders. this has been supported since 1975 by the human science research council and the programme has been extended to include case data on a national basis. this programme has successfully recently been transferred to the human sciences research council. in p'rofessional issues, professor aron has been active throughout her career. she was a student representative of the south african logopedic society in 1952 and the chairman of the first national speech therapy congress in 1953 as well as chairman of its academic activities committee. she has had many periods of chairmanship of the s a speech-languagehearing association (formerly the s a speech and hearing association) as well as editor of its journal (s a journal of communication disorders) and its monthly newsletter.she was elected as president of saslha in 1978. she has been regular chairman of standing committees of saslha to consider ethical, professional and training problems and is currently chairman of the research committee to promote and ' coordinate research in the field in southern africa. a bursary scheme administered by saslha was started in her name for students studying at universities in south africa a very fitting tribute for one who has so long been involved in student issues and concerns. she was a founder member of the council of allied medical professions and was appointed as first chairman of the professional board for speech therapy and audiology of the south african medical and dental council a position she has held from its establishment in 1976 until the present. by request of this board she drew up a memorandum on facilities and training for the j c de villiers committee of enquiry into further facilities for medical, dental and paramedical training in 1984. her international contacts have been very important for the professional in south africa and it is undoubtedly largely due to her efforts and her representation in washington dc in 1980, in tokyo in 1986, at meetings of the international association of phoniatrics and logopedics, together with contact with the american speechlanguage-hearing association from international participation. she has friends and colleagues all over the world and has lectured and travelled in sweden, denmark, britain, canada and the united states. the department and its graduates have an international reputation and the high quality of its training is world renowned. myrtle aron has been a leader in all fields of her profession. the hallmark of her contribution has been a clear and principled direction, a vision and an integrity. she has fought many battles, and has achieved widespread respect. she has done more than anyone to gain the profession its independent autonomous status. she has been an inspiration to all those she has taught and has set the highest standards for energy and dedication. her planned activities during her retirement thankfully mean continued involvement in many aspects such as teaching and research, and it is with gratitude that those of us who have to follow still have her direction, wisdom and experience vto' guide us. ' i list of publications of myrtle l. aron (not including abstracts, reviews, formal addresses, reports, ' memoranda, unpublished conference papers or commentaries) aron, m.l. stuttering therapy an integration of speech therapy and psychotherapy../. s.a. loi/opedic society, 4, 1, 3-8, 1957. bauman, s & aron, m.l. research needs in speech pathology. /. s.a loyopedic satiety, 6, 2, 1-5, 1960. aron, m.l. the nature and incidence of stuttering among a bantu group of school-going children../. speech and hearinq disorders (us). 27, 2, 116-128, 1962. ' x aron, m.l. the effects of the combination of trifluoperazine and amylobarbitone on adult stutters. medical proceedini/s, 11, 10, 227-237, 1965. / aron, m.l., bauman, s. & whiting, d. speech-therapy in the republic of south africa: its development training and organization of services. british /. disorders of "'communication, 2, 1, 78-83, 1967. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) myrtle l. aron aron, m.l. communication problems of the handicapped child. proceedings of the national conference on the hadicapped child, transoranje institute, pretoria, 1967. aron, m.l. the relationship between measurements of stuttering behaviour../. s.a logopedic satiety, 14, 15-34, 1967. aron, m.l. hearing screening and testing in infancy. public health, 72, 11, 335-341, 1972. aron, m.l. pierre de villiers pienaar (festschrift)../. s.a speech and hearing association. 20, 7-13, 1973. aron, m.l. approaches to the pre-school hearing impaired child. in proceedings of the national symposium on the organization of comprehensive autliological services in south africa. department of otorhinolaryngology, university of stellenbosch, cape, 3442, 1974. aron, m.l. communication for the hearing impaired some plain talk. inaugural lecture. witwatersrand university press, johannesburg, 1974. aron, m.l. language communication for the hearing impaired child: a perspective strategy. talk (royal national deaf children's society), 86, 19-22, 1978. aron, m.l. concepts underlying the develoment of a computer information retrieval programme for research in communication disorders. british j. disorders of communication, 16, 2, 89100, 1981. aron, m.l. the development of an information retrieval programme for various communication disorders. in proceedings of the 18th international association of logopedics and phoniatrics congress, washington, d c, august 1980, published as special edition by folia phoniatrica, 1981. anderson, d & aron, m.l. incidence and sex distribution of specific articulatory errors derived from a computerized information retrieval programme. humanitas. 9, 3, 365-374, 1983. aron, m.l. community work and speech therapy. in proceedings of the symposium on the role of the speech therapist in a multilingual society, pretoria july 1984. university of pretoria, 10-16, 1984. 5 aron. m.l. editor. the south african j. of communication disorders, 1971-1980, and 1984-1985. aron, m.l. community speech and hearing therapy: orientation and training. proceedings of the 20th congress of the international association of logopedics and phoniatrics, tokyo, japan: folia phoniatrica 1986. aron. m.l., smit, w. & allsopp, p. w1pcom manual for the computerized information retrieval programme of case data on communication disorders. department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, 1986, isbn 0854949178. aron, m.l., lewis, r.e. & willemse ,j.l. the use of signs and the coding of prefix markers at a school for the deaf. s.a. jnl. communication disorders, 33: 64-72, 1986. aron, m.l. community-based rehabilitation for communication disorders. proceedings of the s.a. speech and hearing association national conference, pretoria, 1987. aron, m.l. data bank of communication disorders. research bulletin, 33-38, 1987. aron, m.l. opening keynote address of audiology congress. proceedings of the audiology congress, cape town, 1988. aron, m.l. communication rehabilitation for the hearing impaired adult with an acquired hearing loss. proceedings of the audiology congress, cape town. 1988. aron, m.l. rehabilitative procedures with the hearing impaired adult. proceedings of the 21st congress of the international association of logopedics and phoniatrics, prague, czechoslovakia. 1989. aron, m.l. implementation of community speech and hearing work in disadvantaged areas. proceedings of the 21st congress of the international association of logopedics and phoniatrics, prague, czechoslovakia, 1989. rosebank hearing aids extra strong to extra small!! suppliers of rexton, bosch, viennatone and microson hearing instruments. in-the-canal, in-the-ear, behind-the-ear, power body aids, tinnitus maskers, bone-conductors and cros instruments available. we have our own laboratory facilities on our premises 15 trupps centre 4 tyrwhitt avenue cnr jan'smuts avenue rosebank johannesburg p.o. box 2 1 1 0 parklands 2121 tel: (011) 8 8 0 4 5 8 4 / 5 fax (011) 8 8 0 7 3 1 1 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) this page is sponsored by the literary group (pty) ltd academic & medical booksellers johannesburg: campus bookshop 34 bertha street 2017 braamfontein westdene bookshop 7 ameshoff street 2017 braamfontein phone: (011) 339-1711 phone: (011) 339-3026 durban: logans westdene 660 umbilo road 1400 durban phone: (031) 253221 ext 22 cape town: westdene rondebosch 18 main road 7700 rondebosch phone: (021) 689-4112 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ^ p p t a n c e of deafness in deaf adolescents: a r e p e r t o r y g r i d s t u d y r t j a c q u e l i n e β a l l a n h n e b . s c . ( l o g o p a e d i c s ) . c a p e t o w n department of speech ^erapy, bjrag^anajhhospital, p.o. bertsham, 2013, ^ m < ! t d v investigated the acceptance and definition of deafness by deaf adolescents. a 6prtorv grid (rg) technique was administered to 27 moderate^ to profoundly r e p ired subjects attending orally directed schools. the responses ehciled indicated that t^siiblects ident'ified with neither deaf nor hearing persons, thereby denying their own rieafnms subjects also described deaf individuals (dis) negatively but maintained tivp self-concepts and displayed inadequate definitions of themselves and deafness^ ^ l d l s have distinct implications for the management of the di, with regard both to* assessment and therapy. results further demonstrated that the rg technique is an effective means to investigate the psychology of deafness. u w d i f s t o d i f ondersoek gehoorgestremde adolessente se aanvaarding en definisie van η nfietd 'n "repertory jid" (rg) tegniek is uitgevoer op 27 matig tot erg gehoort ™ signed english f 1 ,,·! fingerspelling i , etc. figure 1 american sign language continuum (after woodward, 1972). blance between the form of a symbol and the thing it stands for (klima and bellugi, 1979). derived from this formal language and the necessity for communication between deaf and normal hearing individuals, a pidgin sign language (psl) has developed naturally, incorporating elements of both english and asl. like any pidgin language, the reduction and mixing of two languages (in this case asl and english) result in new structures. as the continuum approaches pidgin signed english (pse), grammatical features of asl are replaced by english * the term "deaf' is used throughout this article to denote individuals with a severe or profound hearing loss. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31 1984 @ sasha 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sign language in south africa: some research and clinical issues 7 features such as grammatical markers and word order (baker and cokely, 1980). it is of note that many writers mistakenly confuse pse with pure sign language. a further point of confusion arises when examining manually coded english (mce), another point on the sign language continuum. mce refers to the development of several codes which attempt to represent manually, spoken english syntax, and are hence distinct from either asl or psl. such systems include signed english, linguistics of visual english (love), seeing essential english (see) and the rochester method (usa) and the paget gorman system (u.k. and south africa). such manually coded systems had their origin in the increased awareness amongst educators that for the majority of severely and profoundly deafened children, the oral system alone has failed to teach functional english, either written or spoken, and that communication through a visual mode has many advantages (conrad, 1979). hence the notion of 'total communication' has evolved incorporating manually coded systems as well as lip reading, auditory training, speech teaching, etc. to summarize, there are discrete differences between asl, psl and mce, all of which need to be taken into account by the researcher and clinician dealing with the deaf. while sign language per se is a pure language found among deaf users, within educational contexts (i.e. where the deaf mingle with hearing individuals) there is a strong likelihood of observing psl or a manual system which has been formally introduced as an educational option. in south africa, there is clearly a wide variety of visual language being used by the deaf. according to axelrod (1983) the first people to bring a sign system to this country were the irish sisters over a century ago, but details regarding the nature of this sign language are unknown. due to the discrete educational policies at present existing in the country, many separate systems are probably in existence. for black school children a manual code based on the paget gorman system is used (i.e. al^deaf schools falling underj the control of the department of education and training use,this code). white deaf children by contrast, have to date not been permitted to use any signing system for formal education. the same situation exists for coloured and indian communities. the south african national council for the deaf, clearly riot unaware of the discrepancies in educational policies and the urgent need for sign language research, has recently established a communications committee to investigate the whole issue of sign language in this country. an attempt is being made to establish a 'uniform sign language system in south africa' (viljoen, 1982). a book, 'talking to the deaf (nieder-heitman, 1980) is currently being promoted as representative of signs used by the majority of deaf south africans. this is indeed a praiseworthy preliminary effort in a field so lacking in documentation but we believe that the stand taken is premature. first, little attempt has been made to describe the syntactic structure of the visual language presented in the text. second, there is no clear indication as to the research methods employed to establish the stated 'majority' representativeness of the signs. further, the source of many of the photographs is not local but an overseas one (the gestuno system). finally and most important, it would seem highly likely, in view of the preceding discussion that no single south african sign language exists. no one working within the deaf community of south africa, not least the deaf individuals themselves, can deny the existence of a seemingly rich spontaneous sign language which is used in at least some conversational contexts. what is at issue however is the potential variability of such visual language amongst different users. further, sign language as such, is clearly distinct from the manually coded systems used by some groups and undoubtedly shows considerable divergence according to culture, demography and sociological factors. languages develop, they are determined by history, by culture and by geography, and cannot be artificially constructed. in view of the above, it is, we suggest, essentially impossible and certainly unproductive to hypothesize the existence of a uniform sign language in south africa. as evidence for the above suggestions, some preliminary field work into the use of deaf sign language in south africa will now be described. preliminary experimental evidence the aims of this study* were first to describe the signs representing a number of lexical items used by deaf children attending a white residential school for the deaf in johannesburg in order to determine whether the use of these signs is standard within the school population, and second to determine whether these signs differ from those proposed to be the "standard" south african signs as described by nieder-heitman (1980). method forty deaf subjects ranging between the ages of 5-19 years were selected. subjects were congenitally deaf (loss of 80 db or more in the speech range) and were all boarders at the deaf school, thus ensuring that subjects interacted within the same educational and social environment. twelve lexical items were selected according to the following criteria: — 1) all items were required to appear in nieder-heitman's (1980) dictionary "talking to the deaf'. 2) lexical items were selected in terms of their frequency, use and relevance within the subjects' environment. 3) lexical items thought to be represented by highly iconic signs were eliminated in order to avoid the use of natural gestures. 4) nouns, verbs, adjectives and prepositions were selected. it has been suggested that the verb system of any language forms the core of every utterance (wilbur 1976, p. 439) and it is the nouns, prepositions and adjectives which expand the verb phrase into a meaningful language unit. three words for each part of speech were selected as follows: — nouns : dog, mommy, tree verbs : bathing, jumping, sitting adjectives : yellow, old, happy prepositions : on, in front of, to the results reported here are based on a research project undertaken in the department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, by the third author. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 only single lexical items, out of syntactic context, were investigated. stokoe (1978) suggests that when encountering a new language one should commence by comparing vocabulary items. subjects were individually presented with a picture card and corresponding written stimuli representing the respective signs. all signing was video recorded and analysed according to the following variables:reported laterality age — dominant hand used — correspondence to south african signs (nieder-heitman 1980) data was analysed according to 3 cheremes (equivalent to the phonemes of spoken language) as described by stokoe in wilbur (1976, p. 441):tab (tabula) — location on or near the body where the sign is being made dez (designator) the configuration of the hands sig (signation) — the movement aspect of the hands. all data was tallied according to a percentage. the sign for each lexical item was interpreted as occurring in the majority of ss if it occurred in more than half the group. results table i represents the percentage of subjects using a standard sign together with an indication of their correspondence (or otherwise) with the "south african" sign. this table further demonstrates the dimensions along which the signs differ. results indicate that for the twelve lexical items an average of 75 % of the ss are utilizing a standard system of signs which differ from those described by nieder-heitman as representing the south african signs. the mean percentage of subjects utilizing the signs concurs with the findings of marcowicz (1972) who refers to a study by best (1972) which reported that 78.2 % claire penn, robyn lewis and andrea greenstein of deaf children within an oral school used sign language. the results of the present study also support suggestions in the literature that signs are non-uniform throughout the world (battison et al., 1976). it is of note that the subjects' signs for only two of the lexical items ('old' and 'happy') approximate the socalled hypothesized standard. for the other lexical items, there is a high level of agreement between ss for all except one item ('in front of) where considerable variation occurred. the fact that marked differences occurred between the standard signs used by the deaf subjects and the south african signs, reinforces the viewpoint that the signs of the subjects are not simply dialectal variations of the basic south african sign but completely different entities. six of the signs differed by all three cheremes from nieder-heitman's (1980) hypothesized standard and the remaining four signs by at least one chermee. such variation is further illustrated by the examples in figure 2. since an average of only 6.2% of the subjects used the signs from the manual, it is impossible to conclude that there is any close resemblance between the cheremic configurations of the deaf subjects' signs and those appearing in the manual, at least for the signs studied. the subjects appear to have their own unique system which is not idiosyncratic to each individual but is representative (to differing extents) of the group studied as a whole. certain variations did occur among the subjects but these occurred in a small number of subjects only and were close approximations to the more standard signs. the items on which there was somewhat wider variation were 'jumping', 'sitting' and 'in front of. the former two signs ('jumping' and 'sitting') showed variation from gross iconic gesture involving all four limbs to highly arbitrary signs (i.e. signs where there is no resemblance between sign and meaning (baker and cokely, 1980)). the variations observed as a function of these dimensions are illustrated below (see figure 3). it was possible to see a developmental trend in this data in that younger subjects in the group were more likely to use iconic variations for these two signs. though this could not be table 1 percentage of subjects using standard signs for the lexical items investigated sign percentage of subjects using a standard non-south african sign percentage of subjects utilizing a standard south african sign differences across the cheremic configurations ; occurring in the two sets of signs ; (x indicates where a difference occurs) j sign percentage of subjects using a standard non-south african sign percentage of subjects utilizing a standard south african sign tab (the location on or near the body where the sign is being made) dez (configuration of the hands) sig j (the movement ' aspect of the ' hands) dog 95% 0% x x x mommy 77,5% 12,5% a) x b) x tree 80% 10% a) x x b) x x x yellow 92,5% 0% x x x old ] 0% 92,5% / happy j 0% 92,5% bathing 80% 0% x jumping 70% 10% x x x sitting 90% 0% x x / x on 50% 27,5% x in front of extreme variation 2,5% x x x to 87o/o •0% x the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sign language in south africa: some research and clinical issues 9 lexical sign used by subjects south african "standard" item (after nieder heitman, 1980) dog yellow figure 2 differences between signs used by subjects and those hypothesised to be the south african standard, for two lexical items. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 claire penn, robyn lewis and andrea greenstein iconic-arbitrary jumping hands in neutral position; jumping movement; legs involved sitting sitting movement by bending knees; hands not involved clenched fists bent elbows; bend elbows back as knees are bent hand in 'v' position move hand up and down (as if jumping) on back of opposite hand clenched fists; bent elbow twist wrist backwards figure 3 variations in signs used for lexical items "jumping" and "sitting" as a function of the iconicity-arbitrariness continuum statistically confirmed in the present study (due to the nature of the group studied) this suggested trend does support the idea that signs, just like spoken languge, develop along a specific route (wilbur, 1976). the variability characterizing the sign 'in front of might be explained in terms of the fact that the form of certain signs is dependent on context (klima and bellugi, 1974, p.41). for example according to these authors 'sitting in front of the class' may be signed differently from the 'boy stood in front of the girl'. since in the present study, the stimuli were not presented in a linguistic context, the subjects' own interpretations was not accounted for. this possibly accounts for the observed variations evident for this particular sign. further explanation for variation in certain signs undoubtedly comes from the observation made previously that when the deaf come into contact with hearing individuals, differing modifications of the sign may take place (i.e. a type of pidgin signing). as has been previously mentioned, the fact that the writers investigated only isolated lexical items limited the study in view of the fact that the syntax and rules of these signs could not be studied. however, certain features emerged within the signs used by subjects. these features correspond to those features which serve to make american sign language a syntactic linguistically sound language. they a r e : 1) iconicity and arbitrariness e.g. the signs 'jumping' and 'sitting'. 2) phonology (cheremes) e.g. the tab, dez and sig configuration evident in each sign. 3) symmetry and dominance. 4) the suggestion of a developmental pattern for certain signs. the results of this study have thus indicated that there appears to be a difference between selected signs used by the deaf subjects investigated a sub-group of the south african deaf population, and those signs proposed by nieder-heitman (1980) to be representative of all signs used by various ethnic groups in south africa. the variations that exist among these two sets of signs are not merely simple ones, but variations which are standard and arbitrary and which differ markedly from one another. discussion the above experimental evidence serves to illustrate that, at least for the sample tested, there is a lack of conformity with the signs included in the sign language system proposed to be representative of sign language users in south africa. it therefore seem logical to conclude that in other deaf populations in this country who stem from discrete language groups and educational backgrounds, a similar divergence from the hypothesized standard might exist. such a finding naturally awaits experimental confirmation but is certainly not unexpected in terms of the theoretical and research considerations discussed previously. it does, however raise some central and pressing clinical and research implications for the study of sign language and its variants in south africa. the current attempt to unify south african sign language systems (at different points on the sign language continuum) seems both scientifically and pragmatically questionable. rather what seems indicated is a nation-wide investigation of the existing dialectal variations in south africa and a systematic and academic attempt by linguists, sociologists, anthropologists and members of the deaf community to illustrate that the visual language used by the south african deaf is a unique set of phenomena, as arbitrary as rule-governed, as variable and as rich in cultural and linguistic heritage as any other sign system in the world. the implications arising out of this clearly have relevance to deaf educational policy in south africa. though the writers believe strongly in the benefits of a manual code as a supplement to teaching language to the deaf individual, this manual code will clearly have to depend on the existing lexicon and systems used by each individual deaf group in south africa. the systems which should be adopted will depend on a number of factors and may in fact be fairly similar, or at the other 'extreme, as diverse as the existing spoken languages in this country. hopefully it will not be political policies which will determine the eventual adoption of coding systems into educational programmes. rather, the prime concern should be the criterion of effectiveness to the deaf individual. we have a commitment to serve the community in the best way possible and to tliis end we hope that considerable efforts will be made in the future to investigate this promising yet essentially unexplored field of research. references axelrod, c. chaplain to the deaf, johannesburg. personal communication, 1983. baker, charlotte & cokely, dennis. american sign-language. t.j. publishers, 1980. battison, r.m. & jordan, i. king. "cross-cultural communication with foreigners" in sign and culture. william c. stokoe (ed.). maryland: linstok press 1980. conrad, r. the deaf school child. new york: harper & row, 1979. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sign language in south africa: some research and clinical issues 11 klima, edward, &bellugi, ursula. the signs of the language. harvard: harvard univ. press, 1979. markowicz, harry. "some sociolinguistic consideration of a.s.l." in sing and culture. william c. stokoe (ed.) maryland: linstok press, 1980. nieder-heitman, n. talking to the deaf. pretoria: government printers, 1980. stokoe, w.c.: "the study and use of sign language". sign language studies, 1976, 10, 1-36. stokoe, w.c. sign and culture. maryland: linstok press, 1978. viljoen, ds. editorial in the silent messenger. s.a. national council for the deaf, vol. 5(3), 1982. wilbur, r.b. "linguistics of manual language and manual systems". in communication assessment and intervention strategies. l.l. lloyd (ed.) baltimore: univ. park press, 1976. woodward, j. "implications for sociolinguistic research among the deaf." in sign language studies i, 1972. 1-7. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) danavox unique design, unrivalled technology and exceptional sound quality the 123 series is a completely new generation of small hearing aids combining an anatomically correct design w i t h the latest in technology. the 123 series offers the advantages of: modern, attractive design comfortable behind-the-ear fit convenient controls excellent sound quality and low distortion danavox high degree of reliability gain unaffected by changes in battery voltage d o w n to 1 volt very low battery consumption acoustimed hearing services 3rd floor bosman building eloff street johannesburg tel.: 337-2977 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31 84 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 75 research note bonferroni's bound — a control of significance level errors in speech pathology and audiology research c v kass, ph d (witwatersrand) department of statistics university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg μ marks wahlhaus, μ a (log.) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract many studies in behavioural sciences, such as speech pathology and audiology, involve statistical hypothesis testing. repeated tests are made, for example, of judge reliability in assessing the disorder, or within subject variability, or between subject comparisons over several measures of the disorder or types of treatment. if the error rate of the statistical test is only controlled for each individual test, the overall error rate is magnified and the chance of reporting a significant result where none exists, arises. this paper addresses this potential problem, by noting some common procedures that inherently guard against this pitfall, and suggesting a simple, albeit conservative, solution for other cases. opsomming talle studies in die gedragwetenskappe, soos spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, betrek statistiese hipotesetoetsing. herhaaldelike toetse word uitgevoer, byvoorbeeld, van die betroubaarheid van beoordelaars by die evaluering van 'n afwyking, of intervergelykings van proefpersone ten opsigte van metings van die afwyking of van die tipe behandeting wat toegepas is. indien die foutvoorkoms van die statistiese toets slegs vir elke individuele toets gekontroleer word, word die totale foutvoorkoms vergroot en ontstaan die moontlikheid dat 'n betekenisvolle resultaat opgeteken word waar daa/ in werklikheid geen resultaat bestaan nie. hierdie artikel spreek hierdie potensiele probleem aan deur sommige statistiese prosedures wat inherent teen hierdie valstrik waak, te vermeld en deur 'n eenvoudige, hoewel konserwatiewe oplossing vir ander gevalle aan die hand te doen. / overview the usual research in speech pathology, social, psychological and medical sciences typically advocates a significance level of 5% for reporting results-or theories as being established. that is, for example, if a new therapy is to be deemed better than an established regime, then the statistical analysis of an observed "improvement" (eg. a decrease in the frequency of stuttering) must show that such a result could not be ascribed to natural variation in the subject's stuttering frequency except with a 5% chance. in other words, if the stutterer was tested repeatedly (over many weeks) without the new therapy, only one time in twenty (equivalent to 5% of the time) would he show such a marked decrease in stuttering frequency as evidenced on the single test performed after the new therapy. i from the viewpoint of an individual researcher, a significant improvement of the 5% level is satisfactory as it protects her from advocating a new therapy that is no better than the existing one. an unhappy alternate interpretation is that if a hundred researchers all over the world decided to test this new therapy, then five of them could be expected to detect significant improvement even if the new therapy is ineffectual. this is a consequence of statistical testing in die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 modern times. similarly it is understood that for every hundred journal articles that use statistics and claim an improvement of some technique or difference between two approaches that are significant at the 5% level, it is expected that five such articles will be erroneous — although statistically there is no way of knowing which five they are, nor even if there are precisely five in error. for this reason researchers try to claim significant results at more extreme levels. that is, instead of using the 5% level (implying a one in twenty chance of claiming a false positive), the 1% (one in a hundred), 0.1% (one in a thousand) or more extreme level is used to indicate how small the chance is of an erroneous conclusion by the researcher. there is a problem in being too stringent, namely, if very small significance levels are used, false negatives increase; that is, the smaller the significance level the larger the probability of finding no improvement in a new therapy when in fact it really is efficacious. the above considerations are usually well known to the researcher. less understood are the implications when an individual researcher applies many statistical tests (in contrast to many researchers applying a single test as dis© sasha 1988' r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 g v kass and μ marks wahlhaus cussed above). some variations of this theme will now be discussed. one researcher applying many tests the previous example of testing a new therapy to decrease stuttering can be extended as follows: should the researcher consider the therapy to be conducive to reducing some stuttering behaviours but not others, she could subdivide the types of stutter into a number of auditory categories: gasp, glottal stop, laryngealization ... etc. if twenty categories were for instance decided upon the naive approach would be to apply twenty statistical tests, each at the 5% significance level. a simple extension of the original experiment in which the stuttered words are themselves categorised by the five initial phonemes [f], [s], [t], [m] and [h] and the twenty categories of stutter analysed within each of the five word types would result in one hundred statistical tests. in the latter case the analogy between a hundred researchers each employing one test, and the individual researcher applying one hundred tests, is complete. even if the new therapy is valueless, five of the behaviour/word-type combinations can be expected to show a significant decrease in frequency (i.e. clinical improvement); and further, one of these will even be significant at the 1% level. while publication of false positives due to many researchers working in isolation is accepted (that is, it is not expected of a researcher in south africa to anticipate other researchers in the country or throughout the world when performing her statistical tests, any more than they would take into account her research when performing theirs), it is believed that each researcher should include in her reckoning the other statistical tests that she herself performs, at least within a single research topic. failing "protection" in this way spurious significant results would likely be found. there are a number of ways of keeping the overall significance level (i.e. probability of type i error over several statistical tests) down to a pre-specified level, depending on the situation. one of the simplest and most versatile is to use boole's inequality (also known as the first bonferroni inequality), (feller 1968). in essence in its simplest form it states that if the number of tests to be performed is n, and the overall significance level to be contained is p, then each individual test should be performed at the p/n level. thus, in the first example above where the researcher was about to perform twenty (n = 20) tests each at the 5% (p = 5%) level, each test should have been performed at the 0.25% (p/n = 5%/20 = 0.25% = 1/4%) level, only then could any significant result be claimed to be truly meaningful at the 5% level. equivalently she would ensure that her overall error rate was at most 5% (i.e. overall probability of a type i error is at most 5%), by performing each individual test at the 1/4% level. similarly, if she wished to validly test all one hundred behaviour/word-type combinations at the overall 5% level of significance, each individual test could only be claimed to be significant if it attened the 0.05% (5%/100 = 0.05%) = (1 in 2000) level. such tests (at the 1/4% or 0.05% as appropriate) may be called modified 5% level tests that safeguard against the inflation of the probability of a type i error. indeed, without this modification it is almost sure (99.4%) that the type i error (i.e. claim a false positive) will be committed when performing a hundred tests each at the 5% significance level. one possible difficulty in using boole's inequality as described, is that the necessary statistical tables may not be easily accessible. thus, in the above example where a 0.25% significance level was postulated as providing the required protection, the critical values of the chosen test statistic at the 0.25% level may not be published in commonly used tables or appendices. a statistician may be able to provide a reference to superior tables, or a (possible complicated) procedure either to interpolate in tables or to access a computer approximation. some tables have been generated specifically for use with boole's inequality, eg. bailey (1977) gives tables so designed for use with the various forms of the t-test. another approach is to lower the necessary significance level to a value for which the required critical values are tabulated. this will induce a similar proportional reduction in the overall significance level. thus, for example, if the critical values of the 0.25% level are not available but ;hose for the 0.1% level are (i.e. reduced by a factor of 2.5 from 0.25% to 0.1%), then the use of the latter tables will cause a concomitant reduction in the overall significance level from 5% to 2% (since 5%/2.5 = 2%), a more stringent level. an example involving comparisons of variables suppose the researcher wishes to examine the association between ten visual behaviours (i.e. behaviours that can be observed by eye, eg. a jaw jerk, eye flutter, furrowed brow, ... etc) that are manifested during or just prior to a stuttered word. in this case it may be deemed appropriate to examine a matrix of pairwise comparisons, eg. a matrix of correlations or other measures of association or even 'distances' between pairs (as is used in cluster analysis). in the case of product-moment correlations a statistical package like sas (sas institute inc. (1985)) offers an overall test of whether or not all the pairwise comparisons can be considered insignificantly different from zero. unhappily, rejection of the hypothesis that all the comparisons do not differ significantly from zero, does not indicate which of the comparisons show a significant difference, and so the test, while valuable for certain problems, is incomplete as far as the hypothetical researcher into stuttering behaviours is concerned. j ι the global test can thus only indicate whether further analysis of the correlation severally may be profitable. if the global test is rejected at the 5% significance level, then individual tests may be performed. as before, use of boole's inequality is recommended. ' in the example of ten visual behaviours (or ten judges) there are 45 pairwise comparisons. thus each of the 45 tests should be executed at the 0.1111 % level (5%/45 = 0.1111 %), or perhaps more conveniently at the slightly lower 0.1% level. note that the procedure of considering these 45 tests each at the 0.1% level is a valid method for containing the overall significance level at most 5% for any such matrix of paired comparisons (eg. rank correlations) and not just product-moment correlations. in the field of speech pathology and audiology judge agreement is often a relevant research issue. the assessment inter-judge reliability poses problems with a similar structure to the example which has been examined, i.e. the association at stuttering behaviour. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) bonferroni's bound — a control of significance level errors in speech pathology and audiology research 7 7 an example involving analysis of variance as a final example consider a researcher who measures stuttering frequency on a number of subjects before and after five different therapies (eg. a control group 'time heals' therapy, a fluency-based approach, a stuttering-modification approach, psycho-therapy treatment using psychopharmocological drugs). the null hypothesis that all these methods are equally effective (or equally ineffective) based on appropriate measures, is a standard analysis of variance problem. built into this technique are tests of all the alternative subhypotheses including those of one or two therapies being different to one, two, three or even all the other therapies. like the previous case of correlations examined above, rejection of the null hypothesis does not indicate which of the many alternative sub-hypotheses may be significant. again if the sub-hypotheses of interest (eg. the control group is worse off than any of the others, the drug therapy is better than the others, the last two are better than the first three, ... etc.) can be listed and numbered (let there be η of them) then boole's inequality can be invoked as before to give an overall significance level of p, by conducting each individual appropriate t-test and the p/n level. this approach is, however, only recommended if the number of sub-hypotheses of interest is fairly small (eg. up to η = 4 say). the reason for eschewing boole's inequality for larger η in this case is that more powerful tests have been developed although some require specialised statistical tables. these tests come in various forms and are under such headings as 'multiple range tests', 'multiple comparisons' and 'simultaneous testing procedures'. a discussion of the more popular tests may be found in winer (1971), while a comprehensive review including more modern procedures can be found in miller (1981). standard statistical packages such as sas (sas institute inc. (1985)) offer methods such as duncan's multiple range test, gabriel's multiple comparison procedure, tukey's studentized range test, among others, on request. conclusion it is clear from this study that each researcher should ensure that her overall significance level is controlled within an accepted bound, when performing multiple tests whether implicitly or explicitly. some statistical procedures and their associated computer programs contain such built-in protection, eg. analysis of variance. other techniques unfortunately do not normally provide such a safeguard, nor do the associated computer programs supply a caveat; theoni^and two-sample t-test and their non-parametric counterparts such as the wilcoxon test fall into this class. in these cases the use of boole's inequality as described in this paper is recommended. each researcher should be aware that performing many tests each at an accepted significance level, could lead to an unacceptable increase in the type i error; that is "discoveries" may be made which are, indeed, due merely to chance fluctuations. it is unfortunate that within the purview of behavioural research, the simple protection against this type of error outlined above, is not more widely used. references bailey, b.j.r. tables of the bonferroni t statistics journal of the american statistical association. 469—478, 1977. feller, w. an introduction to probability theory and its applications. wiley, new york, 1968. miller, r.g. simultaneous statistical inference. 2nd ed. springer, new york, 1981. sas institute inc. sas user's guide: statistics, version 5 edition. cary, nc, 1985. winer, statistical principles in experimental design. 2nd ed. mcgraw hill, new york, 1971. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) doit yourself insertion gain instrument if you have a suitable ibm compatible computer and if you are familiar with it, acoustimed can now offer you a "do it yourself" insertion gain instrument. we sell you a kit which you assemble yourself without any special tools. the equipment has all the features of the ha-2000 ii system but is in a less expensive housing and we save on installation costs. any support which you may need is given over the telephone or in our offices — saving you thousands of rands on the world's most versatile hearing aid analyzer. features: complex test signals fast pure tone sweep speech weighted signals transients, bursts, continuous signals built in signal synthesizer real time analysis time delay spectrometry "prescription" calculations are programmable auto-correlation for noise reduction signal averaging and spectrum averaging rms, peak and crest factor displayed linear response probe microphone data management with sophisticated data base program easy to use acodat programming language word processor with graphics facility mailing list programs invoicing programs calendar/scheduling program no other system offers all these features. write or call for a descriptive booklet. n b . this is a marketing experiment for which we have prepared two instruments. we reserve the ! right to request that you bring your computer to us for assembly, demonstration and instruction. i i i acoustimed (pty) ltd. 327 bosman building cor. eloff and bree streets johannesburg tel: (oil) 337-2977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, or critically evaluative theoretical, or therapeutic issues dealing with disorders of speech, voice, hearing or language, or on aspects of the processes underlying these. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. all contributions are reviewed by at least two consultants who are not provided with author identification. form of manuscript. authors should submit four neatly typewritten manuscripts in triple spacing with wide margins which should not exceed much more than 25 pages. each page should be numbered. the first page of two copies should contain the title of the article, name of author/s, highest degree and address or institutional affiliation. the first page of the remaining two copies should contain only the title of the article. the second page of all copies should contain only an abstract (100 words) which should be provided in both english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors. all paragraphs should start at the left margin and not be indented. major headings, where applicable, should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements and references. tables and figures should be prepared on separate sheets (one per table/figure). figures, graphs and line drawings must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper. lettering appearing on should be uniform and professionally done, bearing in mind that such lettering should be legible after a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should not be included in the illustration but should appear below it. the titles of tables and figures should be concise but explanatory. the title of tables appears above, and of figures below. tables and figures should be numbered in order of appearance (with arabic numerals). the amount of tabular and illustrative material allowed will be at the discretion of the editor (usually not more than 6). references. references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and date, e.g. van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, et al. after the first author will suffice. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list. references should be listed alphabetically in triple-spacing at the end of the article. for acceptable abbreviations of names of journals, consult the fourth issue (october) of dshabstra cts or the worldlist of scientific periodicals. the number of references used should not exceed much more than 20. note the following examples: locke, j.l. clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord, 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j . katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology, 3rd ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. proofs. galley proofs will be sent to the author wherever possible. corrections other than typographical errors will be charged to the author. reprints. 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. all manuscripts and correspondence should be addressed to: the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech and hearing association, p.o. box 31782, braamfontein 2017, south africa. inligting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer verslae en artikels oor navorsing, of krities evaluerende artikels oor die teoretiese of terapeutiese aspekte van spraak-, stem-, gehoorof taalafwykings, of oor aspekte van die prosesse onderliggend aan hierdie afwykings. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings sal nie materiaal aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. alle bydraes word deur minstens twee konsultante nagegaan wat nie ingelig is oor die identiteit,van die skrywer nie. formaat van die manuskrip. skrywers moet vier.netjies getikte manuskripte in 3-spasi'ering en met bree kantlyn indien, en dit moet nie veel langer as 25 bladsye wees nie. elke bladsy moet genommer wees. op die eerste bladsy van 2 afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, die naam van die skrywer/s, die hoogste graad behaal en die adres of naam van hulle betrokke instansie verskyn. op die eerste bladsy van die oorblywende twee afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskyn. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming l 100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. alle paragrawe moet teenaan die linkerkantlyn begin word en moet nie ingekeep word nie. hoofopskrifte moet, waar dit van toepassing is, in die volgende volgorde wees: metode, resultate, bespreking, gevolgtrekking, erkennings en verwysings. •tabelle en figure moet op afsonderlike bladsye verskyn (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie). figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge moet oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte gedoen word. letterwerk wat hierop verskyn moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50%-verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by die illustrasie moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verklarings of omskry wings moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maardaaronder verskyn. die byskrifte van tabelle moet bo-aan verskyn en die van figure onderaan. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word (met arabiese syfers). die hoeveelheid materiaal in die vorm van tabelle en illustrasies wat toegelaat word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). verwysings. verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, bv. van riper (1971). waar daar meer as twee skrywers is, sal et al. na die eerste skrywer voldoende wees. die name van alle skrywers moet in die verwysingslys verskyn. verwysings moet alfabeties in 3-spasiering aan die einde van die artikel gerangskik word. vir die aanvaarde afkortings van tydskrifte se titels, raadpleeg die vierde uitgawe (oktober) van dsh abstracts of the worldlist of scientific periodicals. die getal verwysings wat gebruik is, moet nie veel meer as 20 wees nie. let op die volgende voorbeelde: locke, j.l. clinical phonology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j . katz (ed^handbook of clinical audiology, 3de ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice hall, 1971. proewe. galeiproewe sal waar moontlik aan die skrywer gestuur word. die onkoste van veranderings, behalwe tipografiese foute, sal deur die skrywer self gedra moet word. herdrukke. 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis verskaf word. alle manuskripte en korrespondensie moet gerig word aan: die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings. die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraaken gehoorheelkunde, posbus 31782, braamfontein 2017. suid-afrika. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 43 gehoorsiftingsresultate van 'η groep kleurlingleerlinge santie meyer, m(log) (pretoria) marina hurter, b(log) (pretoria) felicity van rensburg, ba(log) (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria. opsomming gehoorsifting wat bestaan het uit suiwertoon en immitansiesiftingsprosedures is uitgevoer op 201 graad i-leerlinge in twee laerskole in die kleurlingwoonbuurt, eersterust. 15,7% van die ore wat getoets is, het nie voldoen aan die kriteria vir gehoor binne normale perke nie. daar is gevind dat 2ψο sensories neurale gehoorverliese en 13,6ψο middeloortoestande vertoon. die kinders uit die lae sosioekonomiese omgewing vertoon 'n geringe verhoogde voorkoms van middeloorprobleme volgens die immitansiesiftingstoetse. as gevolg van die stremmende effek van selfs 'n geringe of fluktuerende gehoorverlies op opvoedkundige vordering, word gereelde oudiologiese dienste vir die skole aanbeveel. abstract auditory screening consisting of pure tone and immittance screening tests was carried out on 201 grade i pupils from two primary schools in the coloured residential area of eersterust. 15,7% of the ears tested did not comply with the criteria of hearing within the normal ranges. it was found that 2 & « . f a g s . l s e g a l o w i t z (ed.), munication in bram-damaged pat ents. ^ y o r ^ l a n g u a g e junctions and brain organisa g o u t s . treasure of ma. new york, w.w. norton and _ £ ° τ 7 μ 1 9 & o'connor, j . k . pragmatic functions in aphasia j. comma, disord., 15, 337-346, 1982. holland, a.l. some practical considerations in aphasia rehabilitation.in m. sullivan and m.s. kommers (eds.), rationale for adult aphasia therapy. nebraska, university of nebraska medical center, 1977. holland, a.l. communicative abilities in daily living. baltimore, university park press, 1980. holland, a.l. observing functional communication of aphasic adults. j. speech hear. disord., 47, 50-56, 1982. holland, a.l. spontaneous recovery from stroke: an investigation of its earliest phases. paper presented at the academy of aphasia, minneapolis, minnesota, 1983. kazdin, a.e. assessing the clinical or applied importance of behaviour change through social validation. behaviour modification, 1, 427-452, 1977. kertesz, a. western aphasia battery. london, ontario canada, university of western ontario, 1980. linebaugh, c.w., kryzer, k.m., oden, s.e. & myers, p.s. reapportionment of communicative burden in aphasia: a study of narrative interactions. in r.h. brookshire (ed.), clinical claire penn aphasiology conference proceedings. minneapolis, mn, brk publishers, 1982. mueller, s.l. an investigation in social competence using clinical and societal profiles. ma thesis, university of california, santa barbara, 1983. penn, m.c. syntactic and pragmatic aspects of aphasic language. doctoral dissertation, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, south africa, 1983. prutting, c.a. pragmatics as social competence. j. speech hear. disord., al, 123-134, 1982a. prutting, c.a. observational protocol for pragmatic behaviours. developed for the university of california, santa barbara speech and hearing clinic, clinic manual, 1982b. prutting, c.a., & kirchner, d. applied pragmatics. in t. gallagher and c. prutting (eds.), pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. san diego, college-hill press, 1983. prutting, c.a., kirchner, d., hassan, p., & buen, p. a societal appraisal of pragmatic behaviours. unpublished manuscript, university of california, santa barbara, 1984. wertz, r.t., weiss, d., kurtzke, j.f., et al., a comparison of clinic, home and deferred treatment of aphasia. veterans administration cooperative study protocol, v.a. hospital, martinez, california, 1978. wertz, r.t. personal communication, v.a. hospital, martinez, california, 1984. wilcox, m.j. aphasia: pragmatic considerations. topics in language disorders, 3, 35-48, 1983. the profile of communicative appropriateness: a clinical tool for the assessment of pragmatics claire penn ph.d (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology & audiology, , university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract i the profile of communicative appropriateness — a newly developed profile for the characterisation of pragmatics is described. the theoretical background to this profile is covered as well as its main components. its application to a group of eighteen aphasic patients is outlined, results suggesting that patient groupings on the profile could be predicted in terms of severity but not in terms of type of aphasia. explanations for this finding are discussed and the potential utility of this profile is suggested. 1 opsomming die profile of communicative appropriateness — 'n nuutontwikkelde profiel vir die karakterisering van pragmatiek word beskryf. die teoretiese rasionaal hieragter en die hoofkomponente van die profiel word behandel. die toepassing hiervan op 'n groep van agtien afatiese pasiente word omskryf resultate dui daarop dat die pasientgroeperings aanduidend kan wees van die erns van afasie maar nie van die tipe afasie nie. verduidelikings hiervoor en die potensiele bruikbaarheid van die profiel word bespreek. the clinical profile is a method of characterising language which has become increasingly popular in recent years. essentially " . . . a linguistic profile is a principled description of . . . those features of a person's . . . use of language which will enable him to be identified for a specific purpose." (crystal, 1982). the format of such a profile is the presentation of a wide range of variables simultaneously so that the clinician is able to see at a glance the communicative assets and deficits of a patient. the main purpose of such a profile according to crystal (1982) is to provide not only a comprehensive description of a patient's data but also an adequate basis for remedial intervention. it is not a standardized measure; nor is it an exhaustive linguistic description. the profile is, however, a compromise to the clinician faced with the realisation that language disability requires comprehensive and individual description. the amount of information contained on a profile is determined by the behaviours being measured © sasha 1985 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) profile of communicative appropriateness : a clinical tool for the assessment of pragmatics as well as the purpose of the tool. however, it is imperative that the categories are clinically relevant i.e. that they can potentially distinguish patients with differing symptoms. the language behaviours which can be characterised by profile are numerous. crystal and his co-workers developed the larsp profile in 1976 to characterise expressive syntax and have subsequently introduced profiles to describe phonology, prosody and semantics (crystal, 1982). prutting has developed a profile (protocol), based on speech act theory designed to characterise pragmatic capabilities (prutting and kirchner, 1983). this paper will describe an alternative pragmatic profile, developed over some years viz. the profile of communicative appropriateness (pc a). the pca is a linguistic profile designed to characterize the communicative competence of a clinical subject. it was developed to identify the features of communication evading description by traditional methods. it is concerned primarily with language use beyond the sentence level and is based on a number of theoretical assumptions from the field of pragmatics. before a consideration of the pca and its application, this theoretical basis will be examined in further detail. theoretical background the field of pragmatics has been defined as the study of " . . .the rules governing the use of language in context" (bates, 1976). according to prutting (1982) "the context in which communication takes place is highly complex and includes multidimensional aspects of the environment". it takes into account the people present in the interaction, what was said before, the topic of conversation, the task of communication and the time and place of the interaction. i in figure 1 the main components of the communicative context are isolated viz. participants, codes, channels, setting and content. t h e c o m m u n i c a t i v e c o n t e x t c o n v e r s a t i o n a l m a x i m s figure 1 the realm of pragmatics although in the field of linguistics and in language pathology, there is at present what prutting (1983) calls a "paradigm shift" towards the field of pragmatics, the area is a complex one and as yet ill defined many linguists have declared a reluctance to incorporate such contextual factors into a model of language, their claim being that such considerations fall outside the realm of linguistics per se. on the other hand, there is ample evidence to suggest that many aspects of communicative competence are amenable to analysis and can be systematically described and related. these aspects have for the most part been dealt with largely independently in the literature and have different frameworks of analysis. aside from the work of bates (1976) there is as yet no cohesive theory of pragmatics which attempts to combine the study of language use into a unified whole. at this stage a discussion of pragmatics therefore probably necessitates a consideration of its component parts. central components selected for discussion here are reflected schematically in figure 1 viz. response to interlocutor, topic control, cohesion, fluency, sociolinguistic sensitivity and non-verbal commu· nication. to separate these particular aspects is a somewhat artificial exercise as there are many areas of overlap. the reasons for considering them separately are governed by their distinction in the literature their relative independence from a methodological perspective, but most important by their potential practical separation as useful areas for the characterization of pathological language. it is beyond the scope of this paper to present in any depth the scope and theoretical underpinnings to the aspects discussed. this has been described elsewhere (penn 1983a). presented here is merely an outline of the main components of communicative competence with their respective definitions. components of communicative competence probably the most important area of control which the normal speaker-hearer has over his language in real life is his competence with regard to discourse. most human communication takes place beyond a sentence level i.e. in dialogue or in conversations. appropriate response to an interlocutor thus involves knowledge of the rules of discourse and an understanding of the speaker's intention with regard to a particular utterance. coherence is a central feature of discourse i.e. the property that makes a discourse more than a collection of unrelated simple sentences. two aspects of coherence in discourse may be identified the first is related to control of topic or of semantic content. keenan and schieffelin (1976) define discourse topic as the proposition about which the speaker is either providing or requesting new information. the rules for topic cooperation are very complex including those for topic shifting, shading (expansion) and reintroduction. the notion of cohesion is the second major component of coherence. this refers to the way in which sentences are linked within a discourse. cohesion may be expressed through the syntax or vocabulary and includes components such as reference, substitution, ellipsis and conjunction, all of which have been discussed at length by workers such as halliday and hasan (1976). fluency is another aspect of communicative competence which has been examined in considerable detail by both linguists and speech pathologists. it is considered as a sensitive indicator of the potency of the communicative system. dalton and hardcastle (1977) point out that there are two possible ways to view fluency. the first emphasizes temporal and sequential aspects of speech and includes factors such as pauses and interruptions. the second meaning of fluency is derived from the context of language usage and includes adherence to the rules of language. clearly in characterizing communication, we are concerned with both aspects, though speech therapists have traditionally concerned themselves more with temporal and sequential aspects of fluency (penn, 1983b). the term sociolinguistic sensitivity coined by bates and johnston (1977) describes the speaker's awareness and sensitivity to the contextual features of his utterance and his ability to modify his message in terms of this context. the speaker who is sensitive to the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 claire penn t .he situation will take into account the a communicatively competent speaker will also show good control and understanding of non-verbal transmission of messages. much has been written during recent years on this channel of communication (behrmann & penn, 1984). it includes areas such as kinesics, proxemics and paralanguage. the function of non-verbal communication changes according to context and the patient's specific abilities. non-verbal communication is in fact an important component of all aspects of communicative competence discussed previously. finally on a more esoteric level, there are certain properties of conversation which determine its overall qualitative level of appropriateness. in order to communicate effectively certain ground rules must be followed by the communicative participants. borrowing from kant, grice (1975) has suggested that four maxims apply in conversation: quantity, quality, relation and manner and that these are basic to the rules of cooperative discourse. by way of a summary of the preceding discussion, figure 1 illustrates the aspects discussed. it demonstrates the primacy of communicative context over the speaker's use of language and the basic influence of the cooperative principles of conversation. it also indicates the separate (but overlapping) areas of communicative competence discussed above. the pca reflects an attempt to capture such aspects for the purposes of clinical description and prediction. a clinically viable version of the pca is presented in figure 2. the six main areas of communicative competence are presented as well as the specific linguistic behaviours subsumed under each scale. the pca evolved from a study designed to investigate the expressive output of a group of adult aphasic patients. it is not the intention of this paper to describe the detailed outcome of this study. what seems important is a consideration of how the pca evolved and its utility in describing and separating out aphasic subjects. date person eliciting sample features of sampling unit of analysis / . t f v / / / contents request 2 ο reply % = = ο clarification request o s. gs acknowledgement s. ζ teaching probe others topic initiation topic adherence ° -s s topic shift ο £ lexical choice £ ε ο idea completion υ " idea sequencing others elipsis tense use c reference •2 lexical substitute forms -c relative clauses υ prenominal adjectives conjunctions others interjections repetitions > » incomplete phrases c ο false starts κ pauses word-finding difficulties others polite forms reference to interlocutor υ placeholders, fillers, stereotypes ts >» acknowl edgements self correction comment clauses sarcasm/humour (λ control of direct speech indirect speech acts others vocal aspects: intensity pitch rate intonation _ ο w 'ζ. quality ϋ .a non-verbal aspects: facial expression ι 3 s ι ζ ε head movement ι 3 s ι ζ ε body posture υ breathing social distance gesture and pantomime others total figure 2 profile of communicative appropriateness the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the profile of communicative appropriateness : a clinical tool for the pca and aphasia the pca evolved in a two-part study conducted over five years. in the first part (part a) the interactive language samples from a group of six aphasics were analysed syntactically using the larsp profile (crystal et al, 1976). this syntactic analysis was found not to identify the main communicative features clinically differentiating the subjects. among such features were factors relating to manner of production, control of content and control over rules of discourse. this led to the derivation of a taxonomy of behaviours designed to represent these aspects. as an audiotaped data base was the basis for analysis, non-verbal communication could not be considered in study a. in study β which employed videotaped analysis of the interactive language samples of fourteen subjects, a non-verbal scale was included. the pca thus measures three broad areas of communicative competence control of discourse (measured by scales α. β and c), fluency (scale d) and more global aspects of interactive communication (scales ε and f). a major concept underlying the pca is the notion of appropriateness the language behaviour of an individual may only be judged as being appropriate within the context of a communicative event. the term "appropriate" implies a societal framework of judgement based on the performance of the individual in a social context, rather than on his ability on an "all or none" measure ot language on a traditional test. in the writer's opinion, the perceived impact of an aphasic patient's difficulty can not be evaluated in terms of a score or a quantitative measure, but in terms of qualitative appropriateness. the issue of how to characterize appropriateness was considered in the present study which used, like holland (1982) and prutting the assessment of pragmatics 21 and kirchner (1983), a dichotomy rating of appropriate/inappropriate in study a. in study β a five-point rating scale was used. in study a, the task of the judges (six qualified and specially trained speech pathologists) was to rate each conversational turn in terms of its appropriateness for each aspect of the pca. a conversational turn was defined as one therapist-patient interaction. difficulties were experienced in analysing the data using statistical procedures for a number of reasons. firstly, the nature of the judgement proved too precise for the nature of the data under evaluation. secondly, the large number of judges, while providing valuable insight into the clinical utility of the tool, provided difficulties in the calculation of inter-rater agreement. as a result, in study b, a five-point rating scale was employed, illustrated in figure 2. two trained judges were required to rate the data of the subjects using the five point scale to evaluate each one minute chunk of language data (designated the conversational unit) in the sample of each of the subjects. inter-rater agreement (measured by means of cohen's weighted kappa coefficient (cohen, 1968) reached acceptable levels for each of the six scales. additional measures for each of the subjects included the administration of the boston diagnostic aphasia examination (bdae) (goodglass & kaplan 1972) holland's cadl (1980), and sarno's functional communication profile (1975). the approach to data analysis was essentially taxonomic making use of a technique known as hierarchical cluster analysis. this allows for the clustering of subjects on a particular measure on the basis of their similarity. the outcome of the cluster analysis can be portrayed schematically by means of cluster fields. in figure 3(a-f) the outcome of the cluster analysis on the separate scales of bdae severity rating (a) high high ο ο ) ο bdae severity rating (b) bdae severity rating (c) high (c 3 £ c 3 bdae severity rating (d) bdae severity rating (e) bdae severity rating (f) figure 3(a-f) clustering of the subjects on scales a-f of the pca in relation to appropriateness and severity ratings (study b) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 claire penn the pca in study β is demonstrated as a function of the subjects' the "la in y reveal that the subjects severity ratings on the buafc. clustered i n t o several distinct groups on the basis of appropriateness the size and the composition of the groups varying according to the scale. on the whole, results suggested a better retained competence for scales α, β, ε and f than for scales c and d. when results for all scales combined are viewed (figure 4) two broad subject splits can be identified — the appropriate group (consisting of eight subjects) and the inappropriate group (containing six subjects). these splits correlated broadly with severity (as measured on the bdae) and with performance on other measures of communication (see figure 5) but not with the subjects' syntactic capacities nor with type of aphasia. high med low 2 3 bdae severity rating figure 4 clustering of the subjects on all scales of pca in relation to appropriateness and severity ratings (study b) below median above median performance on fcp and/or cadl figure 5 comparison of subjects' performance on pca and other communication measures results thus suggest that the subjects showed differential retention of communicative skills as measured on the pca — a finding supported by goldblum (1983) who applied prutting's protocol to adult aphasics. further the results lend support to the idea that traditional methods of classification and assessment may need some reconsideration. possible explanation for results returning to the pragmatic framework presented in figure 1, the results of the present study suggest that within a given communicative context (whose participants, channels, code, setting and context are at least partially specified) aphasic patients' rules for communicating differed from those of normals and between individual subjects. no striking evidence was observed however which suggested that such differences reflected the use of a completely distinct set of rules within any particular group. rather, the patients' capabilities seem to lie on a continuum. discourse, fluency, sociolinguistic sensitivity and non-verbal aspects were assessed in terms of communicative appropriateness. results suggested that communicative competence is often well retained in aphasia and that difficulties pattern those of normals, occurring however with greater frequency. davis (1983) has suggested that "adult aphasia can be understood in part as a disturbance of normal processes". the effect of aphasia appears to be that of limiting or reducing the individual's capacity to apply the conversational maxims effectively. the maxims of quality and relation are properties of conversation concerned with basic truth conditions and. semantic content. control of such matters (particularly in scales a and b) was implicated in certain subjects. quality and manner of production (reflected on scales c and d) are also affected by brain damage. the behaviours measured on scales ε and f might be seen as those strategies employed by the subject to overcome the communicative difficulties imposed by his defect. the fact that there was a considerable degree of overlap between scales and between patients on the scales confirm that communication is after all a global process. the aphasic patient's control of communication and his adaptation to his difficulties reflects his overall communicative competence. utility of the pca with regard to the pca, the results of the study have indicated in a preliminary sense the potential clinical utility of the profile as a measure to characterise aspects of aphasic language use. 'it appears to have fulfilled many of the criteria suggested by crystal (1982) as being necessary properties of a linguistic profile: ι — it will provide at a glance a simultaneous appraisal of the patient's areas of strength and weakness in a communicative sense. — it may be used as a diagnostic or screening measure. for screening purposes only broad areas of communicative competence (e.g. control of semantic content) need be characterised. for diagnostic purposes an in depth appraisal of specific aspects (e.g. topic management) could be explored. — the pca is flexible and by no means finite. it provides an opportunity to code 'other' behaviours and also has a category for unanalysable features, (could not evaluate). further the conversational unit may be varied according to the interest and the needs of the examiner. it may be timeor turn-based. an additional flexible component of the pca is that it can be used to characterize interaction between a patient and any interlocutor, not necessarily his therapist. — although unlike other linguistic profiles developed on children, it is not graded, it does appear to provide a basis for remedial intervention in that it has generated many hypotheses regarding the direction of future therapy for those patients examined . in the present study. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the profile of communicative appropriateness : a clinical tool for finally, although the pca emerged initially from the study of adult a p h a s i c ' l a n g u a g e , this certainly does not preclude its use with other l a n g u a g e i m p a i r e d populations. encouraging preliminary data has e m e r g e d from the application of the pca to a group of head injured subjects (irvine, 1984), a schizophrenic subject (cohen, 1984) and some hearing impaired children (sacks, 1984). further ongoing research exploring the usefulness of the pca in the area of learning disability, dementia and in the clinical supervision of students is being explored by the author. it is hoped that the next few years will see a burgeoning number of studies concerned with the refinement and broadened application of this apparently useful clinical tool. acknowledgements the writer is indebted to professor m.l. aron, head, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand for her valuable supervision of the research on which this paper was based. for financial assistance, the writer is indebted to the human sciences research council and to the senate and council research committees of the university of the witwatersrand. references bates, e. language in context. new york, academic press, 1976. bates', e. & johnston, j.r. pragmatics in normal and deficient child language. short course presented at asha convention, 1977. behrmann, m. & penn, c. non-verbal communication of aphasic patient's. brit. j. dis. comm. 19, 155-168, 1984. cohen, j. weighted kappa: nominal scale agreement with provision for scaled disagreement or partial credit. psychological bulletin 70, 213-220, 1968. cohen, l. the communicative competence of a paranoid schizophrenic. undergraduate research report. dept. speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand, 1984. crystal, d., garman, m. & fletcher, p. the grammatical analysis of language disability. london, edward arnold, 1976. crystal, d. profiling linguistic disability. london, edward arnold, 1982. i dalton, p. hardcastle, w.j. disorders of fluency. london, edward arnold, 1977. the assessment of pragmatics 23 davis, g.a. aphasia and normal adult language processes. paper presented at asha convention, 1982. goldblum, g.m. aphasia: a societal and clinical appraisal of pragmatic and linguistic behaviours. unpublished m. a. dissertation. dept. speech. university of california, santa barbara, 1984. goodglass, h. & kaplan, e. the assessment of aphasia and related disorders. philadelphia, lea & febiger, 1972. grice, h.p. logic and conversation. in p. cole & j.l. morgan (eds.) syntax and semantics. speech acts vol. 3. new york, academic press, 1975. halliday, m.a.k. & hasan, r. cohesion in english. hong kong, longman, 1976. holland, a.l. communicative abilities in daily living. baltimore, university park press, 1980. holland, a.l. observing functional communication of aphasic adults. j. speech hear. dis., 47, 50-56, 1982. irvine, l. the communicative and cognitive deficits following closed-head injury. undergraduate research report, dept. speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand, 1984. keenan, e.o. & schieffelin, b.b. topic as a discourse notion: a study of topic in the conversations of children and adults. in c.n. li(ed.) subject and topic. new york, academic press, 1976. penn, c. syntactic and pragmatic aspects of aphasic language. unpublished doctoral thesis. university of the witwatersrand, 1983. penn, c. fluency and aphasia: a pragmatic reconsideration. s.a. j' comm. dis., 30, 3-9, 1983. prutting, c.a. pragmatics as social competence. j. speech hear dis., 47, 123-134, 1982. prutting, c.a. the pragmatics of language. s.a.j. comm. dis., 31, 3-5, 1984. prutting, c.a. & kirchner, d.m. applied pragmatics. in t.m. gallagher & c.a. prutting (eds.) pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. san diego, college hill press, 1983. sacks, j. some grammatical discourse features in the older hearing impaired child. undergraduate research report. dept. speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand, 1984. sarno, m.t. the functional communication profile. new york, institute of rehab. medicine, 1975. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) medical software and hardware specialities practice accounting packages rehabilitation aids patient history management computers and peripherals also available bureau facilities clinical computer concepts cc ck 85/03210/23 103 highlands north medical centre, cr. louis botha avenue & 3rd avenue, highlands north 2192. south africa. phone (011) 440-9412 for expert advice / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 the provision of speech, language and hearing services in a rural district of south africa judith anne mckenzie rehabilitation unit tintswalo hospital abstract in this paper the delivery of a speech, language and hearing therapy (slht) service in a rural area is discussed. in the light of the need to relate the delivery of this service to principles of primary health care (phc) and community based rehabilitation (cbr), a brief theoretical background is given. obstacles to service delivery are then presented, followed by a description of some attempts to implement principles of phc and cbr. the author concludes that many challenges need to be faced in providing slht services that will benefit the majority of the population of south africa. opsomminc in hierdie artikel word die dienslewering van die spraak-taal-en gehoorterapeut in 'n plattelandse gebied bespreek. in 'n poging om die behoefte aan hierdie dienslewering in verband te bring met die beginsels van primere gesondheidsorg en gemeenskap gebaseerde rehabilitasie, word 'n kort teoretiese oorsig verskaf. probleme tydens die instelling van 'n diens word uitgelig. hierna word die rol van 'n spraak-taalen gehoorterapeut in 'n plattelandse gebied ondersoek, beide binne die rehabilitasie en die primere gesondheidsorg-span. die gevolgtrekking wat gemaak word is dat alhoewel die spraak-taal-en gehoorterapeut 'n belangrike rol het om te vervul in die platteland, hierdie rol gesien moet word in die lig van die gemeenskap se behoeftes. dit is egter 'n groot uitdaging vir spraak-taal-en gehoorterapeute om 'n diens te lewer wat tot voordeel van die meerderheid populasies in suid-afrika sal strek. the profession of speech, language and hearing therapy (slht) is under pressure in south africa to provide services to the majority of the population within a situation of transition. according to aron (1991) the linguistic, cultural and socioeconomic changes in the country will affect the nature, demand and delivery of our professional service. slht services within the public sector are delivered mainly through the health sector under the public service. therefore an examination of theoretical principles of health and rehabilitation policy is necessary in order to formulate how slht can rise to meet these demands. it is nonetheless insufficient to scrutinise the level of policy alone, since it is clear that practical realities dictate the way in which policy decisions can or cannot be carried out. newell (1989, p.80) in his discussion of district health systems stated that: although building blocks of a health service may be designed so that their general goals and form are consistent with a national standard, their expression in the field is not always the same. no countries are homogeneous; differences in structure, wealth, geography, disease pictures, and beliefs may influence how health services are delivered. ideally, this tier conforms to a national standard, and reflects accepted rules of accountability as viewed from the capital, but within each block there should be unique qualities as viewed from the bottom. the "block" that he identifies is the district. this paper can be seen as an attempt to take a view "from the bottom" that is the district level, at the implementation of slht services. it is with this in mind that the present paper is aimed at locating the practical experience of slht in a rural district in south africa within current demands for change arid in the light of theoretical principles. this paper will begin w;ith a discussion of some of the obstacles to providing an slht service in the rural district. this will be followed by a description of several projects which have been implemented in attempts to address these problems. the discussion of these projects will include an examination of the significance of each project with respect to the principles of primary health care (phc) and community based rehabilitation (cbr). in addition tliere is a need to clarify the principles of both-phc and cbr, which provide the guiding principles for the delivery of an slht service in the mhala district of gazankulu. before such a discussion can be meaningful, however, it is important to understand the physical and social background of the district and community under discussion. by relating practical experience to theoretical principles, the author aims to promote discussion of the ways in which the present model of service delivery of slht can be made more relevant to the present needs of south africa.it is clear that this is a major task and that the experience presented here is limited. moreover the issues which are raised, are complex and deserve much more considered debate than can be presented here. taking this into account, it is hoped that this paper can serve as a starting point for debate. sasi.ha th south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ision of speech, language and hearing services in a rural district of\sa 51 description of the mhala district, gazankulu and its community the mhala district of gazankulu is situated in the lowveld area of the eastern transvaal. the population of this area experiences a high degree of poverty and unemployment. despite the lack of documented studies, health authorities have concluded that there is a high degree of undernourishment in the area in both young and old (infraplan, 1992). the community rates lack of water as its major health problem with poor attitudes of health workers and problems with sanitation coming next (nethworc, 1992). the population of mhala is estimated at about 200 000 which is settled in 4 towns and 75 widely dispersed villages. the size of villages varies from 107 to 6 918 people [infraplan, 1992). in addition to this population, there has been an influx of refugees to the district from mozambique. this is estimated by local relief agencies as about 32 000 people (sr. agnes, 1992 personal communication1'. the proportion of the population with speech and hearing disorders is indicated by a survey conducted by the occupational therapy department of the university of the witwatersrand. this survey reveals a rate of 4.16 per thousand for speech disorders and 5.29 per thousand for hearing disorders in the mhala district (concha & lorenzo, 1988). health services in this district are provided by two hospitals (tintswalo, 266 beds in the north and matakwane, 178 beds in the south), two health centres, fourteen fixed clinics and one mobile clinic which serves ten visiting points on a fortnightly basis. there is in addition, considerable overlap with mapulaneng district which is serviced by mapulaneng hospital, and patients from both districts are seen in both hospitals (infraplan, 1992). in 1987, rehabilitation services were implemented at tintswalo hospital. at present the rehabilitation unit consists of one physiotherapist and two physiotherapy assistants, one occupational therapist and three occupational therapy assistants and one speech, language and hearing (slh) therapist and one slh community worker. a training programme for community rehabilitation workers (crws) was initiated in 1991 by the occupational therapy department of the university of the witwatersrand in consultation with the rehabilitation unit at tintswalo. at present nine students are in training who will be based in the community from 1993 (lorenzo, 1991). principles of phc and cbr the phc approach andj health care was formulated at alma-ata in 1978 at a joint world health organisation (who) and united nationsjchildren's fund (un1cef) conference. this approach is based on a definition of health that goes beyond the mere absence of disease, but also involves a state of physical, social and mental well-being (mckenzie, α., 1989). primary health care is defined as: essential health care based on practical, scientifically sound and socially acceptable methods and technology made universally accessible to individuals and families in the community through their full participation and at a cost that the community and the country can afford to maintain at every stage of their development in the, spirit of selfreliance and self-determination (world health organisation (who) and united nations children's fund (unicef), 1978, p.110). the guiding principles of phc are: equitable distribution of health care resources and adequate quality care for all 1 sr. agnes, acornhoek catholic mission. a focus on preventive and promotive health services use of appropriate technology active community participation redressing of socio-economic equalities and use of a multisectoral approach (mckenzie, α., & mazibuko, 1989). in addition to these broad principles, the phc approach also identifies certain essential basic service components as follows: education concerning prevailing health problems and the methods of preventing and controlling them promotion of an adequate food supply and proper nutrition ail adequate supply of safe water and basic sanitation maternal and child aealth care, including family planning immunisation against the major infectious diseases prevention and control of locally endemic diseases appropriate treatment of common diseases and injuries provision of essential drugs (mckenzie, α., & mazibuko, 1989). the basic tenets of phc have been adopted in south africa as health policy. however buch (1989) has noted that the reality of the health care system is far from achieving these goals. he has argued that gross inequity still exists within the system, as indicated by the relatively low health budget for the homelands. preventive and promotive services are still not getting the priority they merit and community participation cannot be a reality in the absence of a broader political democracy. thus the present reality for slht services in south africa within the public sector is that we are operating within a phc policy which is inadequately funded and poorly implemented. slht services will need to take into account both the principles underlying present policy and the obstacles preventing successful implementation. at this point it is useful to consider the relationship between phc and rehabilitation. the declaration of alma-ata states that phc "addresses the main health problems in the community, providing promotive, preventive, curative and rehabilitative services accordingly" (who & un1cef, 1978, p. 112). thus it is accepted that rehabilitation is a component of primary health care. slht, as a component of rehabilitation services in general, can also fee accommodated within the phc model. while phc may provide an overall framework for delivery of slht services, we need to examine its implementation in rehabilitation more specifically. mclaren (1989) has argued that rehabilitation should not be merely seen as the last resort when medical treatment has failed. it should be seen as part of a comprehensive overall health strategy in which disability prevention should be a priority of phc. if this is the case then slht should be an integral part of phc strategy, together with other rehabilitation disciplines. the strategy proposed by the who for disability prevention and rehabilitation is community based rehabilitation (cbr). this approach defines rehabilitation as follows: it includes all measures aimed at reducing the impact of disabling and handicapping conditions, and at enabling the disabled and the handicapped to achieve social integration. rehabilitation aims not only at training disabled and handicapped persons to adapt to their environment, but also at intervening in their immediate environment and society as a whole, in order to facilitate their social integration. the disabled and handicapped themselves, their families and the communities they live in should be involved in the planning and implementation of services related to rehabilitation (who, 1981, p.39). what is striking about.this definition is its emphasis on the person with a disability within the social environment. thus cbr the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 judith anne mckenzie does not aim to develop the abilities of the person with a disability alone but also aims to change the society in which that individual lives so that the effects of disability can be minimised. cbr therefore, aims to deliver rehabilitation as defined above in a manner which builds on the resources of the community. rehabilitation occurs in the community insofar as is possible with referral to hospital where appropriate (mclaren, 1989). in order to obtain community involvement, education of the community is seen as vital. finally, it is acknowledged that the training of new levels of rehabilitation workers is necessary (mclaren, 1989). given the theoretical background, how does this apply to the district under discussion and the practical implementation of a slht service? as discussed above there are obstacles to the provision of phc services which must affect slht too. these obstacles will be discussed below. however, attempts have been made in the face of these obstacles to implement the principles of phc and cbr. examples of projects conducted by the slht service at tintswalo will be presented with a critical discussion of how they succeeded or failed in implementing these principles. obstacles to the implementation of the principles of phc and cbr 1. inadequate infrastructure and poverty. poor telecommunications and roads affect the delivery of slht services. community visits are time-consuming and arrangements have to be made well in advance in person or by letter. support structures and services for the disabled are very limited. thus, for example, the child with a mental handicap cannot be referred to an appropriate agency, but becomes the problem of the slh therapist. as noted previously there is a high degree of poverty in the mhala district. this means that clients for slht can not come in for regular therapy since they are unable to pay for transport costs. in addition, slht disorders can appear to be a trivial concern when one is struggling to survive. these issues relate to the phc principle of redressing social inequality and a multisectoral approach (mckenzie, α., & mazibuko, 1989). it will come asno surprise to the reader that there is much to be done in this regard. 2. training. the training of the slh therapist is not appropriate to the demands of the rural setting. specific skills related to the remediation of slh disorders cannot be used until the problems that prevent people from taking advantage of a slht service are addressed. therapy might thus be provided for eg., the cerebral palsied (cp) child, but if the mother can not bring the child in on a regular basis then the service is ineffective. the therapist needs to develop skills in working with the community in order to find more appropriate ways of dealing with slh disorders. the challenge lies in moving away from those clinical skills where s/he feels confident and secure, into the less certain waters of the community. the slh therapist is trained to deal with sophisticated equipment but is not trained to work in the setting where this equipment is not available. the inadequate budget of rural hospitals has been discussed previously. this is experienced in slht in the absence of audiometric equipment. the slh therapist will find that s/he has little experience of alternative and appropriate ways of testing hearing. in this instance, not only is training inappropriate, but also there is a lack of suitable appropriate technology. 3. community awareness. there is a lack of awareness in the community about the service that we provide. thus campaigns of awareness need to be conducted. the tension here is between raising awareness and developing the service in such a way that the increased demand can be dealt with. it is the impression of this author that this tension can be resolved to a certain degree by the manner in which awareness is raised. werner and bower (1982) have described two approaches to health education. the first is authoritarian and controlling and creates dependency and feelings of helplessness. the second is what they term "people-centred" learning. this helps people to become stronger and more self-reliant. if awareness campaigns for slht services are conducted in the first manner it is possible that the service might be overloaded, since people have not been encouraged to help themselves. they will depend on the service to do that for them. if the second approach is used it is possible that the service offered could be seen as one way to approach the problem once community efforts have failed. 4. the balance between hospital and community. there has been some difficulty in prioritising hospital and community services. how much time should be spent in the hospital and how much in the community? at present the rehabilitation service is between two models. institution based rehabilitation, which occurs within a hospital setting is the old model (mclaren, 1989). it is obviously a valuable service and it is what the slh therapist is employed at present to provide. however, as an attempt is made to implement cbr there is a necessity to be in the community more and more. support for working in the community from head offices and the posts for community work are lacking. description of projects attempting to implement principles of phc and cbr despite the obstacles noted above, attempts have been made within the rehabilitation service at tintswalo to implement the principles of phc and cbr". selected lessons from this for slht will now be discussed. 1. survey on stuttering. it was noted that very few stutterers were being referred to tintswalo hospital for treatment (five cases in three years). this led to the consideration that either stuttering was not perceived as a problem, or that it did not in fact occur in this community on any significant scale. it was in order to test this possibility that a survey of attitudes, knowledge and beliefs about stuttering was done in the acornhoek/timbavati area (mckenzie, 1992). the community rehabilitation worker students planned and carried out the survey as part of their training. a hundred and thirteen community members were interviewed: fifty-two of these were primary school teachers. the respondents were selected by means of stratified random sampling. the results can be summarised as follows: 1 91% of respondents knew what stuttering is 2 93% of respondents knew at least one person who stutters ! 3 86% of respondents believed that it is not infectious 4 77% of respondents believed that stuttering causes problems for the individual 5 87% of respondents believed that stuttering cannot be cured 6 68% of respondents believed that stuttering cannot be prevented 7 26% of respondents believed that stuttering is caused by rain falling on the child while still a baby 8 42% believed that it is hereditary or "in the person's nature". from the above, it is clear that stuttering is considered a problem in the community, but as long-as people believe that it cannot be prevented or cured, there is no reason to consider referral for speech therapy. however, it does appear that stuttering could be a problem for the individual. this was confirmed in an awareness campaign arising from the survey. it south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) provision of speech, language and hearing services in a rural district of sa 53 became apparent that many children were being heavily penalised in schools because of their fluency problems. in certain cases children who were able to achieve scholastically, had stopped coming to school because they were not able to cope with the punishment or teasing. the conclusion that can be drawn from the above is that the slh therapist is likely to have little success with either recruiting or treating individuals who stutter. even if stuttering therapy did take place the chance of success would be limited, given the prevailing beliefs and attitudes relating to stuttering. the definition of rehabilitation within cbr, emphasising the need to effect changes in the environment of the person with a disability is relevant here. the child who is penalised for fluency problems at school requires not only assistance with his/her speech but also an attempt to change the negative attitudes of the people around him/her. the strategy for effecting changes in attitude is community education. this forms an integral part of cbr. such education has been given to the teachers in report back sessions on the survey and in a subsequent workshop. teachers have stated that this education was useful to them, since they had not understood the problem previously. they suggested that the focus should now be placed on the parents. it is envisaged that this will be the next phase of the awareness campaign. the above example gives validity to the prominent place that community education is given within cbr this education should ideally be supplemented by appropriate and effective remediation of the fluency disorder. 2. workshop for disabled children. another feature of cbr is the involvement of families in the rehabilitation process, as discussed above. in 1989 a workshop for mothers of cerebral palsy (cp) and mentally handicapped (mh) children was run by the rehabilitation unit of tintswalo hospital (mckenzie, j & concar, 1991). the motivation to run the workshop arose from visits to a clinic of tintswalo hospital. since this clinic is 80 kilometres away from the hospital it was only possible to visit once a month. this resulted in lack of progress in the children and inadequate understanding of the mothers about the children's problems (mckenzie, j. & concar, 1991). the aim of the workshop was to provide a comprehensive educational programme addressing the mothers' and children's needs. thus the physiotherapy department gave input on handling and positioning the child. the occupational therapy department educated the mothers about play, toy making and activities of daily living. the slht department focused on feeding and communication.| assessments of each child were done by all members of the team together and home programmes were given involving all therapies. the programme did not only focus on rehabilitation techniques. there was health screening, and health education talks on nutrition and family planning were given. one of the most successful sessions was where mothers shared their feelings and experiences of having a disabled child. mothers were fully involved in this programme at every stage which was a valuable experience, as they became much more confident in handling their children. however, as this workshop was held at some distance from the village where they lived, it was not possible to involve other family members sufficiently. another important feature of this workshop to be highlighted was the significance of working as a rehabilitation team. this enabled the therapists to assess the person with a disability as a whole rather than from the point of view of their own discipline. this aspect was developed through combined assessment, where problems in order of priority for the mother were emphasised rather than those relating to specific disciplines. while the workshop itself was very successful according to parent and staff evaluations, the long term results are not satisfactory. the group continued to meet sporadically after that time but were unable to formulate any combined goals. there remained a great deal of dependency on the rehabilitation unit. ultimately the project was discontinued. possible reasons for this failure were discussed among rehabilitation staff. there was consensus that local community structures had been inadequately developed. the mothers continued to depend on the rehabilitation unit because there was little support for them in the community. the principle of cbr's building on local resources (mclaren, 1989) had not received due attention. in addition the rehabilitation effort had not been an integral part of the local phc programme and thus support was also lacking from this quarter. it appeared that this lack of support could be attributed to inadequate development of local community structures. one of the positive results of this effort was the selection for training as a crw of one of the mothers of a disabled child in this group. she is at present trying to revive the group. it will be interesting to see whether she, as a community member and a mother of a disabled child with specialised training will be more successful in this project. while this example and the previous one, look at approaches to already existing disability and ways of minimising its effects, the following example is concerned with prevention of disability as an integral part of the phc approach. 3. hearing screening. in the past, screening of hearing in creches and preschools has been conducted by the slht department. problems in covering all the schools due to lack of human resources were experienced. therefore it was decided that education of teachers about hearing loss should be undertaken, thereby encouraging appropriate referrals. teachers in creches were given input on the causes of hearing loss, how to identify the child with a hearing loss and where to refer such a child. this programme, however, was also unsatisfactory, for the following reasons. the slh team was acting in isolation from the phc team. thus the phc team did not understand our goals and the follow up medical treatment was not always satisfactory. referrals to the clinic were not always acted on by the parents because of lack of money and lackof community awareness of the problem. furthermore teachers felt that they were not in a position to motivate the parents to attend the clinic because their own knowledge was scanty. it was therefore decided to incorporate hearing screening as part of a comprehensive programme of health screening in creches. the screening programme is a joint venture of the health services development unit of the university of the witwatersrand and the rehabilitation unit and school health services of tintswalo hospital. health sciences students (medical, occupational therapy and physiotherapy students) were given the task of screening under the supervision of a trained member of staff. in liaison with the senior hospital staff, it was agreed that referrals from such screening would be seen free of charge at the hospital and clinic. creche teachers were consulted on how such a programme should be run. the clinic sister in the area where screening takes place was informed on that day and was given a list of those children who have been referred to the clinic. to date, the programme has not been formally evaluated but it is intended that this will be done soon. informally the response from teachers and nurses has been very positive. with respect to slht we have detected a marked improvement in the parents' response to referral, now that the first consultation is free of charge. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 judith anne mckenzie such a screening programme is aimed both at preventing hearing loss from occurring and at detecting hearing loss early so that appropriate intervention can be taken. within the framework provided by mclaren (1989) it is clear that the slht service has been operating at the level of primary prevention in the programme by educating teachers. according to mclaren (1989) this entails preventing the occurrence of impairments, in this case hearing loss by education of key figures. the screening programme, however operates at the level of secondary prevention, where ear infections can be treated thereby preventing subsequent hearing impairment. it is clear that both levels need to be addressed. at present, screening only is being done and the education aspect needs to be incorporated into the programme. mclaren (1989) has argued that prevention should be included as a specific objective in phc programmes. this screening programme is aimed at meeting this objective with what appears to be some degree of success. conclusion the above discussion serves to illustrate the many challenges that the delivery of an slht service in a rural district poses. it is hoped that these will be taken up by the profession in a meaningful way so that the majority of the population of south africa can be better served. finally it needs to be noted that this cannot be achieved without the full participation of the community within the service: we must not fall into the trap of professional arrogance. as kaseje (1991, p.2) has warned: doctors and "medical experts" are like roosters they think the sun will not rise, that health will not happen if they do not crow. this attitude of medical arrogance needs to change and there must be more listening and trusting of the primary movers in phc, that is the community. acknowledgements the support of the rehabilitation unit at tintswalo hospital and the community rehabilitation worker training programme are gratefully acknowledged. the author also wishes to thank andrew mckenzie for his assistance in the preparation of this paper. references aron,-m.e. (1991). perspectives. the south african journal of communication disorders, 38, 3-11. buch, e. (1989). primary health care in south africa. nursing rsa, 4(11), 34-36. concha, m. & lorenzo, t. (1988). the prevalence of disability in a rural area of south africa. paper presented at the 2nd european congress for occupational therapists, portugal. infraplan (1992), survey commissioned by mhala health action group. kaseje, d.c.o. (1991). community empowerment: the key to health for all. paper presented at namibia national primary health care workshop. lorenzo, t. (1991). strides in community rehabilitation worker training in a rural area in south africa. world federation of occupational therapists bulletin, 2(4), 2-5. mckenzie, a. & mazibuko, r. (1989). what is primary health care? nursing rsa, 4(11), 30. mckenzie, j. & concar, a. (1991). report on a residential workshop for the mothers of cerebral palsy and mentally retarded children run by the rehabilitation unit of tintswalo hospital. communiphon, 299, 17-19. mckenzie, j. (1992). survey conducted on attitudes to stuttering in acornhoek, mhala district, gazankulu. unpublished. mclaren, p. (1989). rehabilitation and primary health care. paper presented at community rehabilitation meeting, gazunkulu. newell, k. (1989). the way ahead for district health systems. world health forum, 80-87. geneva: who. nethworc education project (1992). first community forum report. acornhoek. who/unicef. (1978). primary health care: report ofthe.international conference on primary health care, alma-ata, ussr. geneva. who. (1981). disability prevention and rehabilitation. technical report series 668. geneva. werner, d. & bower, b. (1982). helping health workers learn (pp. 1.11.30). palo alto ca: hesperian foundation. abacus h e a r i n g s e r v i c e s services available / dienste beskikbaar hearing aid assessment/ bepaling van gehoortoestelle maintenance / instandhouding manufacturers of 'in the ear' vervaardiging van 'in die oor' gehoorhearing aids, earmoulds, swimplugs apparate, oorstukkies, swemproppies repairs & servicing to all makes of herstel en diens van alle soort hearing aids gehoorapparate p.o. box 146276 brackengardens tel: (011) 867-1903 1 4 5 2 helping others to hear better ( 0 1 1 ) 8 6 4 1 9 2 0 / 1 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 3.9, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η spektrografiese ontleding van die vokale van afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremdes santie meyer m(log) (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming die vokaalproduksie van 'n groep kongenitaal gehoorgestremde seuns is spektrografies ontleed. die dowe sprekers toon 'n gehoorverlies van 90db gehoorpeil ofswakker in die beste oor, terwyl die hardhorendes 'n gehoorverlies van 70-90db vertoon. resultate is met 'n normaalhorende groep vergelyk. die spektrografiese analise toon aan dat daar statisties betekenisvolle verskille tussen die dowes en normaalhorendes is ten opsigte van vokaalduur (dowes verleng die duur) die fundamentele frekwensie en eerste formantfrekwensie (dowes verhoog dit) en die tweede formantfrekwensie wat deur dowes verlaag word. die hardhorendes verskil ten opsigte van fundamentele frekwensie en die eerste formantfrekwensie van die normaalhorendes. die implikasies van hierdie bevindings word vir terapie bespreek. abstract the vowel production of eight profoundly deaf, ten severely hearing-impaired, and ten normal hearing subjects was subjected to spectrographic analysis and compared. results indicate significant differences between the profoundly deaf and normal hearing subjects in respect of vowel duration, fundamental frequency, and first and second formant frequencies. significant differences relating to fundamental frequency and the first formant frequency were observed between the severely hearing-impaired and normal hearing subjects. implications of these results regarding speech therapy for the hearing-impaired are discussed. the importance of the ability to correctly articulate vowels is not only that vowels are individual phonemic entities and serve as the basic building blocks of morphemes but also that: 1. vowels — particularly at their onset and their end convey to the listener crucial consonantal information; 2. they contain important prosodic information through variations in fundamental frequency, intensity, and duration. (monsen, 1976) die belang van die korrekte produksie van vokale vir suksesvolle kommunikasie blyk duidelik uit die aanhaling. die feit dat gehoorgestremdes se spraakkommunikasie dikwels oneffektief is, is tot 'n groot mate toe te skryf aan hul onvermoe om vokale akkuraat te produseer (markides, 1970). swak spraakverstaanbaarheid en spesifiek die onsuksesvolle vokaalproduksie kan verklaar word na aanleiding van die sensoriese omstandighede waaronder die gehoorgestremde spraak aanleer (monsen, 1983). die gehoorgestremde se afhanklikheid van visie, vibrasie, propriosepsie, terugvoering van horende volwassenes en die distorsie van die beskikbare gehoor in die aanleer van spraak, lei tot verskille in fonologiese organisasie. verder word die vokale geleer deur bewuste visuele, vibrotaktiele en ouditiewe nabootsing. die artikulasie van spraakklanke is dus stadig en aritmies. die gehoorgestremde kind boots ook die visuele beeld van die spraakklank na en ignoreer die tongbewegings wat nie sigbaar is nie. hul poog ook om die artikulatoriese terugvoer in die produksie van sekere klanke te verskerp om 'n meer spesifieke mikpunt te kan verkry (monsen, 1983). dit is gevolglik redelik duidelik waarom die vokaalproduksie afwykend is, tog is daar 'n gebrek aan basiese inligting ten opsigte van die aard van die afwykings spesifiek in afrikaans. hierdie studie poog om as vertrekpunt te dien in hierdie behoefte aan inligting. om vokaalproduksie egter sinvol te deurskou is dit van belang om 'n objektiewe metingswyse te gebruik omdat dit in die meeste gevalle selfs vir 'n opgeleide luisteraar moeilik is om die die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 oorsaak van 'n gehoorgestremde se spraakfoute te identifiseer (monsen, 1978). om die rede word vokale spektrografies ontleed. die parameters wat akoesties ontleed is, is die eerste twee formante wat die belangrikste leidraad in vokaalpersepsie is teenoor onder andere die fundamentele frekwensie en vokaalduur wat van sekondere belang is (monsen en engelbretson, 1983). daar is reeds in studies van amerikaanse engels aangetoon dat laasgenoemde twee parameters van belang is in die spraak van die gehoorgestremde en daarom is dit ook in hierdie studie ingesluit (monsen 1974, angelocci, kopp en holbrook, 1964). metode proefpersone agtien tienderjarige seuns met 'n kongenitale sensories-neurale gehoorverlies groter as 70db gehoorpeil in die beste oor is as proefpersone geselekteer. tien seuns het 'n ernstige (70-90db) gehoorverlies en 8 het 'n totale gehoorverlies (groter as 90db) vertoon. daar word vervolgens na eersgenoemde groep as hardhorendes en na laasgenoemde groep as dowes verwys. slegs seuns bo 15 jaar is geselekteer sodat hul laer fundamentele frekwensie akkurate spektrografiese metings kan verseker (huggins, 1980). al die proefpersone het oor 'n normale intelligensie beskik en geen ander fisiese afwykings getoon nie. almal is reeds op voorskoolse stadium met gehoorapparate gepas en daar is toe reeds met opleiding begin. verder is al die seuns se huistaal afrikaans en hulle is almal in 'n orale-ourale opleidingsprogram. tien normaalhorende seuns vergelykbaar met die gehoorgestremdes ten opsigte van intelligensie, ouderdom, huistaal en afwesigheid van enige ander fisiese of spraakprobleme is geselekteer om as kontrolegroep op te tree. materiaal en apparaat twaalf afrikaanse vokale is geselekteer, naamlik (i, y, ε, e, a:, φ, oe, 3, o:, a, a:, u). die vokale is in 'n kvk lettergreep met © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 santi meyer ( 9 ) as inisiele en (p) as finale konsonant geplaas. die konsonante is geselekteer met die oog op die geringste effek op die vokaalformante (angelocci et al., 1964) sowel as duidelikheid op die spektrogram en gemak van produksie vir dowes. ter wille van die natuurlikheid van die produksie is die toetswoord in sinskonteks geplaas. die opnames is gemaak deur middel van 'n nakamichi 550 en is spektrografies deur middel van 'η vii voice identification spectrograph ontleed. prosedure elke spreker het die 12 sinne in 'n toevallig geselekteerde volgorde gelees waartydens dit op band opgeneem is. die prosedure is na 'n tydsverloop herhaal wanneer die spreker die sinne in 'n ander toevallig geselekteerde volgorde gelees het. dit is ook 'n derde keer herhaal. hierna is die bande voorberei vir spektrografiese analises. van elke vokaal is 'n wyeband (300 hz) spektrogram verkry waar die eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies sowel as elke vokaal se duur en fundamentele frekwensie bepaal is (meyer, 1984). resultate en bespreking die gemiddelde duur van die vokaalproduksie van normaalhorende en gehoorgestremde sprekers volgens figuur 1 is 'n eerste uitstaande kenmerk van die resultate dat die gemiddelde duur van die normaalhorendes se vokale opvallend korter is as die van die dowe sprekers. die dowes verleng hul vokale twee tot twee-en-'η-half keer relatief tot die gemiddelde vokaalduur van die normaalhorendes. hierdie verskil in vokaalduur is dan ook statisties betekenisvol op die 5%-vlak van betekenis (scheffe se meervoudige vergelykingstegniek) (steyn, smit en du toit, 1984). sprekers vokaal g e m · d u u r in m.sek. <0 80 120 160 200 210 280 duur in milusekondes figuur 1 die gemiddelde vokaalduur van normaalhorende, hardhorende en dowe sprekers alhoewel die gemiddelde duur van die hardhorendes se vokaalproduksie ook gering relatief tot die duur van die normaalhorendes verleng is, is die verskil nie statisties beduidend nie. die hardhorendes verskil ook betekenisvol op die 5%-vlak van betekenis van die dowes ten opsigte van vokaalduur (scheffe se meervoudige vergelykingstegniek) . 'n tweede opvallende eienskap van die resultate is dat die duur van die onderskeie vokale onderling verskil. dit blyk duidelik uit die resultate van die normaalhorende groep dat daar onderskei kan word tussen kort vokale (a, ce, u, i,) halflang (ε, a, o) en lang vokale (o: y, e, 0, a:) waarvan hul duur 63-72 ms, 81 ms en 112-150 ms onderskeidelik is. hierdie duurverskille word deur die hardhorende sprekers behou, want hul gemiddelde vokaalduur val in dieselfde kategoriee as die van normaalhorendes. in die geval van die dowe sprekers is die duurgrense van die verskillende vokale onderling nader aan mekaar. wanneer die vokale van die kortste na die langste vokaal gerangskik word, blyk dit egter dat die dowe sprekers tog ook sekere vokale korter as ander produseer. die halflang vokale verdwyn sodat die vokale in 'n groep met 'n relatief kort duur (a, 3, ε, ce, a, i, u) en 'n relatief lang duur (o:, 0, a:, y, e) verdeel. die dowe spreker se kenmerkende verlengde vokaalduur is reeds opgeteken (calvert, 1961). hierdie studie bevestig dat dit ook die geval is by die afrikaanssprekende dowe. alhoewel die dowe wel in staat is om vokaalduur waar te neem, is die produksie daarvan uitermate afwykend. 'n moontlike rede is dat die duur van vokale dikwels slegs 'n sekondere onderskeidende element is (monsen en engelbretson, 1983), of/en die dowe spreker nie die belang van die korrekte duur insien nie. daarbenewens kan dit moontlik ook die gevolg wees van die dowe spreker se moeisame spraakpogings omdat sy spraak dikwels onder bewuste beheer is (ling, 1976). in teenstelling met die dowe sprekers het die hardhorende sprekers in hierdie studie nie probleme met die korrekte beheer van vokaalduur getoon nie. die beskikbaarheid van meer gehoorreste het dus 'n ingrypende effek op die korrekte vokaalduur. 'n tweede kenmerk wat uit die resultate na vore kom, naamlik die onderlinge verskille in duur tussen die verskillende vokale, is algemeen in afrikaans beskryf (van wyk, 1977). die inherente duurverskille tussen soortgelyke vokale is deel van 'n normale spreker se linguistiese vermoe (monsen, 1974) waarmee betekenisverskille in afrikaans teweeggebring kan word, byvoorbeeld /pers/ (drukkery) teenoor /pe:rs/ (kleur). die feit dat die vokale in afrikaans in hierdie studie in kort, halflank en lank verdeel kan word, hou waarskynlik verband met die spesifieke linguistiese omgewing waarin dit geproduseer is. koopmans-van beinum (1980) het dieselfde in nederlands bevind, maar met die verskil dat die halflang vokale soos 'n kort vokaal in spontane spraak funksioneer. i i aangesien die duurverskil nie behoue bly in alle spraaksituasies nie, verklaar dit waarskynlik waarom die dowe sprekers nie so 'n onderskeiding in hul spraak tref nie. die meer uitgesproke verskille tussen lang en kort vokale bly egter in 'n mate in hul spraak behoue. die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van die vokaalproduksie van normaalhorende en gehoorgestremde sprekers 'n eerste uitstaande kenmerk van die resultate in figuur 2 is die verskil in die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie tussen die normaalhorende en die gehoorgestremde sprekers. die gehoorgestremde sprekers se fundamentele frekwensie is ooglopend hoer as die van die normaalhorende groep. dit geld vir sowel die dowe as die hardhorende sprekers. hierdie verskille in gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie is statisties betekenisvol tussen die normaalhorenthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η spektografiese ontleding van die vokale van afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremdes sprekers vokaal fo gem. in h z . 100 110 120 130 u 0 150 gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie in hz figuur 2 die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie (fo) van vokale soos deur normaalhorende en gehoorgestremde sprekers geproduseer de en beide groepe gehoorgestremdes. dit is bepaal deur 'n ewekansige blokontwerp (blokke is vokale) as variansieanalisetegniek. die twee groepe gehoorgestremdes, naamlik die dowes en hardhorendes, verskil egter nie betekenisvol van mekaar deur middel van sheffe se meervoudige vergelykingstegniek nie (steyn et al., 1984). i 1 i • ' i tweedens blyk dit dat vokale in figuur 2 verskil ten opsigte van die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie. wanneer die vokale gerangskik word van die met die laagste gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie na die met die hoogste gemiddelde fundamentelefrekwensie kom daar redelike goeiej ooreenstemming tussen die drie groepe sprekers voor. by al die groepe is (a:, o, a) die vokale met die laagste gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie en (0, y, i) die met die hoogste gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie. die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van normaalhorende manlike sprekers in hierdie studie vergelyk redelik goed met ander studies waar die sprekers in dieselfde ouderdomsgroep val. die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van 126hz is vergelykbaar met schneiderman en kryski (1978) se sewentienen agtienjarige seuns se gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van 130hz en peterson en barney (1952) se volwasse manlike sprekers wat 'n gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van 132hz vertoon. die hoer gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van die dowe en hardhorende groep is ook in ooreenstemming met gepubliseerde navorsing in ander tale uitgevoer. die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van angelocci et al., (1964) se 11-14-jarige seuns het met 43hz van die normaalhorendes verskil. in hierdie studie verskil die normaalhorende sprekers met 18hz van die dowes en 24hz van die hardhorendes. die geringer verskil in die resultate van hierdie studie is moontlik toe te skryf aan die feit dat angelocci et al. se proefpersone meer onstabiliteit van die fundamentele frekwensie toon as gevolg van hul ouderdom (11-14-jarige seuns) wanneer laringeale veranderings as gevolg van puberteit voorkom. aangesien beide groepe gehoorgestremdes 'n betekenisvol hoer gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie as die normaalhorendes toon, kan die afleiding gemaak word dat dit die gevolg is van die teenwoordigheid van die gehoorverlies. die afleiding word dan ook ondersteun deur monsen, engelbretson en vermula (1979) se navorsing by gehoorgestremdes met vergelykbare graad van verlies as die sprekers in hierdie studie. the control of fundamental frequency is especially dependent upon the control of vocal fold tension and profound hearing impairment may hinder or prevent a speaker from learning the precise phonatory consequences of the muscular gestures which maintain or alter vocal fold tension in the production of speech. (monsen et al., 1979) die onvermoe om die fundamentele frekwensie te beheer, hou verband met die feit dat twee belangrike kwaliteite van fonasie, naamlik duur en intensiteit, beide waargeneem kan word deur vibrasie, maar nie fundamentele frekwensie nie. gehoor is dus nie so belangrik in die aanleer of beheer van eersgenoemde twee nie. toonhoogte ofte wel fundamentele frekwensie kan egter nie op enige ander wyse as deur middel van gehoor waargeneem word nie (monsen et al., 1979). die gevolgtrekking bied dan 'n gepaste verklaring veral vir die hardhorendes wat probleme toon met beheer van fundamentele frekwensie, maar nie enige moeite ondervind met die korrekte beheer van vokaalduur nie. alhoewel die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van die gehoorgestremde sprekers afwyk van die norrnale, is dit interessant dat die vokale wat 'n relatief hoe en relatief lae fundamentele frekwensie by die normaalhorende groep toon, eweneens dieselfde eienskappe by die gehoorgestremdes toon. dit is 'n belangrike faset van vokaalproduksie in die sin dat die intrinsieke fundamentele frekwensie van die vokaal die struktuur van die spraakmeganisme reflekteer (koopmans-van beinum, 1980). alhoewel die volgorde ten opsigte van die gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie by die gehoorgestremdes nie identies is nie, is daar 'n onderskeid tussen die vokale met 'n laer en die met 'n hoer fundamentele frekwensie wat vergelykbaar is met die normaalhorendes se resultate. die fundamentele frekwensie neig om te verhoog namate die vokaalhoogte toeneem. bespreking van die eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies van normaalhorende en gehoorgestremde sprekers eerstens toon die resultate in figuur 3 dat 'n bepaalde vokaal in die geval van die normaalhorende sprekers in 'n spesifieke frekwensiegebied voorkom. die gemiddelde eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensie dui die verskil tussen die hoe en lae vokale sowel as die vooren agtervokale duidelik aan in hul ligging in figuur 3. die vokale waar daar veral opvallend oorvleueling voorkom, is (i), wat met (y) oorvleuel en so ook (e, 0) en (a, ce). hierdie vokale word egter slegs onderskei ten opsigte van die labiale modifikasie van die mondholte (van wyk, 1977) en die oorvleueling is dus verstaanbaar. slegs 'n geringe verlaging van die eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies word deur lipronding veroorsaak. tweedens, in die geval van die hardhorende groep (figuur 4) is elke vokaal steeds opvallend in sy eie frekwensiegebied gelee, alhoewel die hoe en middelhoe voorvokale en die hoe en middelhoe agtervokale nie so ver van mekaar gelee is as wat by normaalhorendes die geval is nie. dit is duidelik dat daar meer oorvleueling van verskillende vokale se formantfrekwensies is. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 santi meyer hz 3250 3200 3150 3100 3050 3000 29s0 2900 2050 2600 2750 2700 2650 2600 2550 2500 2150 2100 2350 2300 2250 2200 2150 2100 2050 2 0 0 0 1950 1900 ism 1000 1750 1700 1650 1 6 0 0 1550 1500 u50 1400 1j50 1>00 1250 1300 1150 1100 10 50 1000 iso •00 • 50 t o o 750 700 650 600 if· : ι y : q i ? " . »© .> v * : » , ' (j ψ * τ • a η ' · * * mi 1 \ · ^ > * · λ i f • · . . y • % f ? f » «v t f 4 : . *3> jr * at γ « λ s · · •h { • «v 150 200 250 300 350 100 150 500 550 600 650 700 750 600 mo 900 mo hz eerste formant figuur 3 die eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies van vokale geproduseer deur normaalhorendes hz 3250 3200 3150 3100 3050 3000 2950 2900 2850 2000 2750 2700 2650 2600 2550 2500 2150 2100 2350 2300 2250 2200 2150 2100 2050 2000 ί 9 5 0 1900 1050 ιβοο 1750 1700 1650 1600 1550 1500 1150 1100 1350 1300 1250 1200 1150 1100 1050 1000 950 300 050 000 750 700 650 600 550 « φ · , ι ® / © ? ® 4) . v « fc «· · t # . λ * . . * *® j· φ r" : $ 150 200 250 300 350 100 iso 500 550 600 650 700 750 βοο 850 soo »bo hz eerste f0rm4nt figuur 4 die eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies van vokale geproduseer deur hardhorendes derdens, wanneer die resultate van die dowe groep bestudeer word (figuur 5) is die bogenoemde verskynsel nog meer opvallend. die gemiddelde eersteen tweedeformantwaardes is in" 'n klein frekwensiegebied na aan mekaar gelee. (die gemiddelde eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies van die vokale van die drie groepe sprekers verskyn in die bylaag). laastens bied die statistiese verwerkings van bogenoemde formantfrekwensies interessante resultate. die gemiddelde eersteformantfrekwensie van die drie groepe sprekers verskil in elke geval betekenisvol van mekaar op die 5%-betekenispeil wanneer 'n ewehz 3250 3200 3150 3100 3050 3000 2950 2900 2850 2800 2750 2700 2650 260} 2550 2500 2450 2400 2350 2300 2250 2200 2150 2100 2050 2000 1950 1900 1850 1800 1750 1700 1650 1600 1590 1500 u 5 0 ; u00 ι 1350 1 1300 ! 1250 1 1200 1150 i 1100 1050 1000 950 900 650 600 550 150 200 250 300 350 400 450 500 550 600 650 700 750 800 850 900 680 mz e e r s t e formant figuur 5 die eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies van vokale geproduseer deur dowes kansige blokontwerp as variansieanalisetegniek gebruik is, naamlik scheffe se paarsgewyse vergelykingstegniek (steyn et al., 1984). geeneen van die gehoorgestremde groepe se gemiddelde eersteformantfrekwensie is dus vergelykbaar met die van die normaalhorendes nie, en verder verskil hulle ook onderling. wanneer figure 3, 4 en 5 bestudeer word, blyk dit ook duidelik dat die eersteformantfrekwensies van die hardhorende groep hoer gemiddelde waardes toon as die van die normaalhorende groep, veral vir die voorvokale. dit is nog meer opvallend by die dowe groep. wat die tweedeformantfrekwensie betref verskil die normaalhorende groep betekenisvol van die dowe groep op die 5%-betekenispeil. verder verskil laasgenoemde sprekers ook betekenisvol van hardhorende sprekers op die 5%-betekenispeil. die verskil tussen die hardhorende en normaalhorende sprekers is egter nie statisties beduidend op die 5%-betekenispeil nie. die resultate is verkry deur middel van 'n ewekansige blokontwerp as variansieanalise-tegniek (scheffe se paarsgewyse vergelykingstegniek) (steyn et al., 1984). : i weereens is die bevindings ten opsigte van die verskille tussen die normaalhorendes en gehoorgestremdes duidelik wanneer figure 3, 4 en 5 bestudeer word. die gemiddelde tweedeformantfrekwensies van die normaalhorende groep sprekers toon opvallend heelwat hoer en laer waardes as die van die dowe groep. dieselfde waarneming kan gemaak worcl in die geval van die hardhorende en dowe sprekers. wanneer die gemiddelde formantfrekwensies van die dowe en hardhorende groepe met die van die normaalhorendes vergelyk word, is die beperkte omvang van die waardes opvallend. die frekwehsiegebied waarbinne die gemiddelde eersteformantfrekwensie by normaalhorendes gelee is, is 390hz (hoogste waarde 628hz en laagste waarde 238hz). hierteenoor is dieselfde waardes 433hz by die hardhorende sprekers (714hz en 281 hz respektiewelik) en 134hz by die dowes (657hz en 523hz). hieruit word die beperkte omvang van die eerste formant se frekwensiegebied by die dowes duidelik. wat verder in die verband opval, is dat die frekwensies the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η spektografiese ontleding van die vokale van afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremdes 55 die eerste formant van die middel en agtervokale van normaalhorendes verteenwoordig. die bevinding ondersteun dus boone (1966) se opmerking dat die dowe spreker tydens vokaalproduksie sy tong te laag en ver na agter posisioneer. verder is die hoe voorkoms van neutrale vokale soos deur luisteraars in dowes se spraak gei'dentifiseer ook verstaanbaar aangesien die dowe heelwat verskillende vokale in die frekwensiegebied van die neutrale vokaal se eerste formant produseer (angelocci et al., 1964). die betekenisvolle groter frekwensiegebied waarin die hardhorendes se eerste formant voorkom, kan moontlik die gevolg wees van die hardhorende se poging om deur middel van die eerste formant tussen die vokale te onderskei. die eerste formant is vir hardhorende sprekers relatief duidelik hoorbaar en dit is moontlik dat hulle deur groot artikulatoriese/fisiologiese bewegings poog om op die wyse tussen vokale te onderskei — soortgelyk aan die sprekers van angelocci et al., (1964) wat vokaal verskille deur middel van die fundamentele frekwensie wou aanbring. die gemiddelde tweedeformantfrekwensie van die normaalhorende groep wissel oor 'n frekwensiegebied van 1754hz (2624hz van die hoogste vokaal na 870hz van die laagste vokaal). hierteenoor is dieselfde waardes van die hardhorendes 1082hz (2170hz van die hoogste na 1088hz van die laagste vokaal) en die dowes 393hz (1628hz en 1235hz respektiewelik). hieruit is dit duidelik dat die hardhorendes 'n redelike goeie omvang in die gemiddelde tweedeformantfrekwensie toon, maar dat die dowe se tweedeformantfrekwensie in die gebied van die neutrale (a) van die normaalhorende spreker gelee is. die bevinding verklaar weereens die hoe voorkoms van neutrale vokale in die dowe se spraak soos deur normaalhorendes beoordeel. die feit dat die gemiddelde eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies van verskillende vokale so na aan mekaar gelee is en daar dus heelwat oorvleueling in die individuele waardes is, spreek dus vanself dat vokaalvervangings dikwels deur luisteraars gehoor word. daar kan ook uit die oorvleueling van waardes afgelei word dat die dowe sprekers nie akkurate plasing van die artikulators bemeester het nie — veral as die geringer mate van oorvleueling van die normaalhorendes in gedagte gehou word (angelocci et al., 1964). i die normaalhorende groep se gemiddelde eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies toon veral van sekere vokale oorvleueling van waardes. dit is dan veral opvallend jin die geval van (i) en (y) en (e) en (0). die resultate is egter verstaanbaar aangesien lipronding in die produksie van hierdie vokale die enigste veranderlike is wat verskil (van wyk, 1977). j i gevolgtrekking na aanleiding van die bespreking van die resultate kom enkele implikasies vir die oudioloog na vore. ten opsigte van vokaalduur aangesien vokaalduur selfs vir 'n persoon met beperkte residuele gehoor waarneembaar behoort te wees (ling, 1976) en dus waarskynlik ook meer effektief gebruik behoort te word, moet redes vir die afwykende vokaalduur nagevors word. effektiewe terapiestrategiee moet beplan word sodat die dowe sprekers vokaalduur korrek kan aanwend. dit sal bydra tot meer effektiewe dekodering van die boodskap aangesien vokaalduur inligting bevat oor die suprasegmentele eienskappe van klem en dus ook spraakritme sowel as spraakspoed. dit verskaf ook inligting oor segmentele eienskappe, naamlik inligting ten opsigte van 'n aangrensende konsonant sowel as die inherente eienskappe van die vokaal self (monsen, 1976). ten opsigte van fundamentele frekwensie 'n belangrike aspek om in gedagte te hou is dat die gehoorgestremde, maar veral die dowe, nie in staat is om fundamentele frekwensie as gevolg van sy gehoorverlies effektief te reguleer nie. om die rede is dit dus sinvol om vir die spreker die ontbrekende inligting deur middel van 'n ander sensoriese kanaal aan te bied waarop hy wel die veranderings ten opsigte van fundamentele frekwensiebeheer kan waarneem. verskeie navorsers het bewys dat die beheer van fundamentele frekwensie effektief deur middel van die visuele kanaal aangeleer kan word — veral as die linguistiese belang van fundamentele frekwensiebeheer aangetoon word (phillips, remillard en pronovost, 1968). ten opsigte van die eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies tydens vokaalaanleer is dit noodsaaklik dat die spraakopleier seker moet maak dat die dowe inligting sal verkry ten opsigte van tonghoogte en tongposisie. die inligting wat deur middel van die eerste en tweede formante oorgedra word, is of nie vir die gehoorgestremde beskikbaar weens die graad van sy verlies nie, of dit is so ondergeskik aan ander ouditiewe inligting dat hy dit nie benut nie. deur middel van spesifieke ouditiewe of taktiele strategiee kan hierdie inligting egter aan die dowe verskaf word sodat hy ook sy produksie daarvolgens kan rig (ling, 1976). bedankings die skryfster erken met dank die finansiele ondersteuning van die raad van geesteswetenskaplike navorsing. verwysings angelocci, α., kopp, g. en holbrook, a. the vowel formants of deaf and normal hearing eleven to fourteen year old boys. j. speech hear. disord., 29, 156-170, 1964. boone, d.r. modification of the voices of deaf children. the volta review, 68, 686-692, 1966. calvert, d.r. some acoustic characteristics of the speech of profoundly deaf individuals. ongepubliseerde d(phil) verhandeling, stanford universiteit, 1961. huggins, a.w.f. better spectrograms from children's speech: a research note. j. speech hear. res., 23, 19-27, 1980. koopmans-van beinum, f.j. vowel contrast reduction. an acoustic and perceptual study of dutch vowels in various speech conditions. amsterdam: academische pers b.v., 1980. ling, d. speech and the hearing impaired child: theory and practice. washington, d.c.: alexander graham bell association for the deaf, 1976. markides, a. the speech of deaf and partially hearing children with special reference to factors affecting intelligibility. br. j. disord. commun., 5, 126-140, 1970. meyer, s.e. die vokaalproduksie van die afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremde. ongepubliseerde m(log) verhandeling universiteit van pretoria, 1984. monsen, r.b. durational aspects of vowel production in the speech of deaf children. j. speech hear. res., 17, 386-398, 1974. monsen, r.b. normal and reduced phonological space in the production of english vowels. j. phonetics, 4, 189-198, 1976. monsen, r.b. toward measuring how well hearing impaired children speak. j. speech hear. res., 21, 197-219, 1978. monsen, r.b. general effects of deafness on phonation and articulation. in hochberg, i., levitt, h., osberger, m.j. (eds.). speech of the hearing impaired. research, training and personnel preparation. baltimore: university park press, 1983. monsen, r.b. en engelbretson, a.m. the accuracy of formant frequency measurements: a comparison of spectrographic analysis and linear prediction. j. speech hear. res., 26, 89-97, 1983. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 santi meyer monsen, r.b., engelbretson, a.m. en vemula, n.r. some effects of deafness on the generation of voice j. acoust. soc. am., 66, 1680-1690, 1979. peterson, g. barney, h. control methods used in a study of the vowels. j. acoust. soc. am., 24, 175-184, 1952. phillips, n.d., remillard, w., bass, s. en pronovost, w. teaching of intonation to the deaf by visual pattern matching. am. annals deaf, 113, 239-246, 1968. schneiderman, c.r. en kryski, j.a. fundamental frequency change in preand post adolescent deaf males and females. j. am. audit. soc., 4, 64-68, 1978. steyn, a.g.w., smit, c.f. en du toit, s.h.c. moderne statistiek vir die praktyk. 3de uitgawe pretoria: j.l. van schaik, 1984. van wyk, e.b. praktiese fonetiek vir taalstudente: 'n inleiding. pretoria: heer drukkers (edms) bpk., 1977. bylaag die gemiddelde eersteen tweedeformantfrekwensies van die vokale van drie groepe sprekers vokale formante normaalharddowes horendes horendes dowes i fl 238 281 523 f2 2624 2170 1629 y fl 228 280 543 fl 2517 1959 1550 e fl 345 357 504 fl 2329 2098 1692 0 fl 331 357 520 (2 2264 2004 1548 ε fl 397 456 546 f2 1150 1952 1613 oe fl 475 537 541 (2 1300 1513 1354 3 fl 502 526 530 f2 1453 1572 1515 a fl 628 680 630 f2 1057 1202 1404 a: fl 575 715 657 f2 954 1213 1409 d fl 435 513 546 fl 873 1043 1209 0: fl 362 419 458 fl 1055 1137 1181 u fl 272 302 474 fl 870 1088 1235 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gsi 28 auto tymp the gsi 28 auto tymp provides testing capability for tympanometry, ipsilateral and contralateral acoustic reflex testing and screening audiometry. selection of test sequence is as simple as pressing a button! the auto tymp is lightweight and compact so it can be easily moved from one location to another. an optional carrying case is available if more portability is required. t h e n e e d i e r w e s t d e n e o r g a n i s a t i o n ( p t y ) l i m i t e d in association with / in medewerking met hearing and acoustic instruments (pty) ltd lewis's hearing centre (pty) ltd engineered acoustic products noise control 1st floor, 74 george avenue, sandringham 2192. johannesburg, south africa. telex: 4-25028. p.o. 28975, sandringham 2131, south africa. tel: (011) 640-5017. cables: needlerog. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 43 indigenous healers and stuttering rozanne platzky and joan girson department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract traditional beliefs and attitudes of black south africans to stuttering were investigated. four indigenous healers (ihs) from different ethnic groups were interviewed about their beliefs as to cause and management of stuttering, as well as the outcome of their treatment. the data reveals varying degrees of concern about stuttering. the traditional beliefs of cause and management of stuttering show some similarities to current beliefs held by speech pathologists. implications in terms of direction in therapy, cooperation with ihs and future research in this field are discussed. opsomming die tradisionele opvattings en houdings van swart suidafrikaners ten opsigte van hakkel is ondersoek. onderhoude is met vier tradisionele genesers van verskillende etniese groepe gevoer om hulle geloofsoortuiginge te bepaal ten opsigte van die oorsaak en behandeling van hakkel, sowel as die resultate watgekry word met hulle behandeling. die data toon wisselende grade van besorgheid oor hakkel. die tradisionele geloof rakende die oorsaak en behandeling van hakkel stem ooreen met die huidige menings wat gehuldig word deur spraakpataloe. die implikasies van bevindinge word bespreek met behulp van terpeutiese riglyne, samewerking met tradisionele genesers en verdere navorsing. indigenous healers and stuttering an important aspect of stuttering in relation to research and therapy is the attitude^ of the stutterer himself and that of his associates toward his speech symptoms. (ammons and johnson, 194'4, p.39) although emotions and attitudes are highly individualistic, cultural beliefs and background will invariably influence them as "... culture is the indispensable factual background in relation to which the worker adapts his contribution to the situationj before him." (fenalson, 1952, p.4). culture implies meanings, ideas and values that constitute a way of life that pervades relationships, systems of belief and behaviour. it may be assumed therefore that culture will influence attitudes towards stuttering as well as its development and treatment. wendell johnson (1944), in his studies of american indians, was one of the first to investigate cultural influences on stuttering. he found no evidence of stuttering and no name for it was present in the languages of the groups studied. he interpreted this lack of a name as being the reason why stuttering did not exist, because it implied that stuttering was not significant in this culture. later analysis (wingate, 1972) identified methodological problems in johnson's research and his results were discredited. subsequent studies, cited by van riper (1971) found stuttering among population groups as diverse as the japanese and eskimo, leading him to conclude that stuttering exists universally. therefore, the cause is unlikely to be related to cultural attitudes. he does, however, cite kluckhohn (1954) who states that "... impressive differences in the degree and incidence (of stuttering) suggests cultural influences are operative." (van riper, 1971, p.9.) wingate (1972) attributes this to the differences in the importance and expectations of a child's speech, across cultures. this sentiment is reiterated by snidecor (1947), leith & mims (1975), leith (1986) and shames (1989) who identify cultural factors such as stress on speech performance, and child-rearing practices which are likely to influence the incidence of stuttering. having concluded that cultural beliefs influence the incidence of, and attitudes towards stuttering, and that these attitudes should be identified and explored in therapy, it is evident that the speech therapist needs to be familiar with the cultural beliefs of her clients. shames (1989) states that "... therapy becomes an intercultural collision of values, attitudes, expectations and definitions". owing to the disproportionate ratio of black speech therapists to blacks in south africa, one is faced with a situation where the majority of trained therapists are unfamiliar with the cultural backgrounds of their clients. current trends in therapy derive from america and europe with an orientation not designed to meet the needs of cultures as diverse as that of black south africans. the success of therapy may therefore be hindered by the denial of, or failure to acknowledge a client's cultural beliefs through lack of knowledge, and his being forced to conform to those proposed by modern theories. refuting beliefs about cause and treatment may serve only to aliendie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 ate the client (leith & mims, 1975). fenalson (1952) demands that any professional worker attempting to help others in areas of personal adjustment, needs to understand an individual in terms of his culture and its effect on his responses to life experiences. south african psychologists have become increasingly aware of the necessity to re-evaluate the therapy that they offer in terms of cultural appropriateness. holdstock (1979, p.119) goes as far as to say that "... professionals would never be able to satisfy the emotional needs of people", while the therapy that they offer is tailored to westernised cultures. it may be meaningless and ineffectual when applied to members of a culture so starkly different from it. the writers have, therefore, undertaken to study an aspect of the attitudes and perceptions of a sample of black people in south africa to stuttering. in order to attain an understanding of traditional cultural beliefs it was decided to focus on indigenous healers (ihs) for this information as they have been described as "... psychologist, physician as priest... tribal historian" (holdstock, 1979, p. 119). the ih is a highly respected member of the tribe, a reservoir of traditional beliefs and one who has the power to modify customs (hammond-tooke, 1989). he is that indispensable member of the society who is consulted by an estimated 70% of the black population of s.a. (mzinyathi, undated, p.144). in 1977 the soweto society for marriage and family life concluded that the majority of people in soweto believe in the power of the ih (holdstock, 1979). the world health organisation (who) estimated that ihs form the essential core of primary health workers for nine tenths of two billion rural dwellers in third world countries (holdstock, 1979). once urbanised, a black person may well consult an ih as hammond-tboke (1974) predicts that an urban diviner's clientele consists of traditionalists, professionals, middle-class blacks and even whites. ihs may, therefore, be considered a valuable source of information on cultural beliefs about stuttering as well as on the traditional treatment for stuttering. according to hammond-tooke (1989) there are two distinguishable types of healers, i.e. diviner and herbalist. the herbalist is one who has not been mystically called but is a master of medicines. he is one who has knowledge of plants and roots. the diviner, on the other hand has been described by hammond-tooke (1989, p. 104) as one "... clothed with power and knowledge ... called to the profession by the prompting of the ancestors". he, therefore, likens the diviner to a doctor, and the herbalist to a pharmacist. a diviner is consulted in the case of a longlasting illness in order to establish a cause and a remedy for the illness. the diviner was, therefore, considered to be the more suitable subject for this research report. south africa is moving towards a new political dispensation with revised health policies. the inclusion of the ih into these policies is now under debate. the role of the ih cannot be ignored as he is accessible to and consulted by the majority of the black people in south africa. it is estimated that there are between 100π000 and 200π000 ihs practising in s.a. today (freeman, 1992). in the best interests of the clients, krober (1990), therefore recommends fostering a spirit of co-operation between traditional healers and medical practitioners. holdstock (1979) agrees with this recommendation and admonishes members of the helping professions that"... although indigenous healing is as old as the civilisation of africa and at present rozanne platzky & joan girson time adhered to by countless numbers of individuals, people in the helping professions and academic community know next-to-nothing about it and acknowledge it even less" (holdstock, 1979, p. 118). there are those (motlana, 1992) who, on the other hand, are vehemently opposed to any co-operation. they perceive ihs as dangerous due to their lack of knowledge of physiology and anatomy. motlana (1992) criticises any attempt to co-ordinate traditional and modern practices as he feels that it traps the south african black person in a previous era while the rest of the community moves forward into a technologically advanced and sophisticated century. south african speech therapists cannot make an assessment about the efficacy of traditional methods until they have a knowledge of what they comprise. effective, efficient and meaningful therapy may then only be provided once the client's attitudes and expectations have been assessed. methodology aims the aims of this study were twofold: 1. to probe the cultural beliefs and attitudes of ihs to stuttering. 2. to investigate whether they treat stuttering clients and, if so, what this treatment would comprise. subjects because of time and other practical constraints the size of the sample had to be restricted to five subjects. an attempt was made to choose subjects reflecting the population of the ihs which is divided into the nguni, sotho, venda and tsonga groups (hammond-tboke, 1989). unfortunately, no venda subject was available. two subjects had rural, and three urban practices to reflect possible differences in approach. all subjects were diviners, as it was considered that they would be the more likely ihs to be consulted in the event of stuttering. ! all subjects were contacted through the african national healers association (anha), as freeman ;(1992) cautions against consultation with ihs who may not be authentic. for a description of the individual subjects please see table i. j ι procedure i i the survey was conducted through interviews because of the "richness and spontaneity of information" (oppenheim, 1966, p.32) which is obtained through this method of data collection. interviews yield a high response rate and decrease the number of "don't know" and "no answer" responses (young, 1966). the flexibility of an interview was felt to be particularly appropriate in this instance as english was not the home language of any of the subjects and it was therefore sometimes necessary to rephrase, explain and probe. interviews do not require reading or writing ability on the part of the subjects. this was an important consideration as according to the african national healers association (unpublished) not all ihs are literate. an interview is prone to a number of sources of error the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) indigenous healers and stuttering table 1. description of subjects 45 subjects professional name years in practice rural/ urban calling training experience 1. tsonga (female) mungome 22 r fell ill. couldn't eat. no improvement till consulted mungome who told her it was a calling. 8 months with mungome. communicated with ancestors in visions. specialist in childhood health problems. others infertility, insomnia due to witchcraft. ulcers. trains novices. 2. s. sotho (female) ngaka 22 r became ill. taken to ih. inaugurated via rituals slaughtering goats. drinking or smoking medicines. specialist in childhood disorders diarrhoea, vomiting, swollen feet, sores under tongue, nail biting, failure to speak. trains novices. 3. xhosa (male) igquira 7 u fell ill. untreatable by orthodox medicine. 1 year under ih. gained ability to communicate with ancestors and to predict events before they occur. treats mental disturbance, body sores, visual defects, stomach ache, female problems, birth difficulties. 4. zulu (male) inyanga 9 u fell ill. called to profession by his deceased grandfather. 1 year and still consults his trainer. throws bones to formulate diagnosis. ability to cure vd, nausea, vomiting and bewitching given as a few examples. and its scientific utility is limited. it does, however, offer a means to establish contact with the subjects and to assess the appropriateness of this source of information. question construction j the order and sequencing of questions was controlled (young, 1966). most questions were open-ended as these provide the best opportunity to obtain the maximum amount of information from each question. guidelines outlined in the literature (babbie, 1973) were followed in the construction of questions. pilot study following a pilot study, conducted with a southern sotho ih, several changes were made to the sequence and content of the questions. at the end of the interview the respondent asked if she could ask the interviewer some questions. this proved to be valuable as her questions provided insight into her frame of reference and beliefs about stuttering. this procedure was then incorporated into the interview framework. interviews it was felt that the respondents would feel more relaxed and discuss their thoughts and practices more openly in familiar surroundings. subjects 1 and 2, who practice in a rural area, were interviewed at a private residence of their choice in the area in which they live. the other three subjects were interviewed at the offices of the anha. the interviewer followed the subjects' lead in terms of where to sit, i.e. at a table or on the floor. none of the respondents could speak english well and they were, therefore, joined by a member of the african national healers association (not an ih himself) who acted as interpreter. kahn and cannel (1957) caution against the use of an interpreter as it creates a barrier in the establishment of a rapport between respondent and interviewer. in this case it was felt that the interpreter actually set the respondents at ease and facilitated more open responses. an interpreter should merely be a medium through which questions and answers are transmitted (young, 1966). in order to ensure this, sources of error such as prompting or leading the respondent's answers were discussed with the interpreter before the first interview. he showed a knowledge and understanding of the protocol for research. during the third interview, the interviewer became aware that the interpreter was leading the subject. when this interview was analysed by a sotho speaking speech therapy student her suspicions were reinforced. it was, therefore, decided to exclude this subject from the study. following this the process was rediscussed with the interpreter and he was reminded of his role before completing the remaining two interviews. none of the subjects was present at interviews other than their own and they were asked not to discuss their interviews with the other subjects. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 analysis of the data the contents of the interviews were reported upon as "... precise summaries of the data" (forcese and richer, 1973, p.213). as this research was descriptive, results were categorised according to the questions, tabulated, and discussed qualitatively in order to compare the salient features. results and discussion the subjects' views on stuttering, i.e. name, cause, management, outcome and attitudes are presented in table 2, and will be discussed below. name and description of stuttering all the ihs had names for stuttering, which corresponded with those found by aron (1966). si and s2 also gave alternate names. si (tsonga) originally spoke of treating "lilele", which from her description may or may not have referred to stuttering. however, when asked to translate "stuttering" she used the term "konkoretsa" and demonstrated this as syllable repetitions. it is interesting to note the similarity between this word and "korakoretsa" the term used by s2 (south sotho) as these two languages are not related. all the names used were onomatopoeic as are the names for stuttering in many languages, e.g. "tuhuhtuhuh" egyptian, "gimgeim" hebrew and "howdodo" ghana (van riper, 1971). the ih's descriptions of stuttering all corresponded with the description of characteristic stuttering symptoms, i.e. syllable repetitions, complete blocks and sound prolongations given by peters and guitar (1991). table 2. summary of data rozanne platzky & joan girson cause all 4 subjects identified stuttering as an inherited disorder, although s4 was originally undecided. authorities in the field of speech therapy have, over the years, researched heredity as a causative factor. theorists now seem to agree that certain constitutional factors may be inherited and predispose a child to stuttering (van riper, 1973; bloodstein, 1987). 52 and s3 gave additional causes which seem to have their roots in folklore, i.e. being left out in the first rains of spring or failure to inform the ancestors of the child's imminent birth. s2 gave witchcraft as another alternative. these have no parallel in the professional literature. diagnosis and management the subjects differed in their approaches to diagnosis and management. si believes there is no treatment for konkoretsa as it is hereditary. s2 treats according to the cause. where the cause is hereditary there is usually a sore under the tongue which she treats with medication. if the stutter is caused by being left out in the rain the child must be treated outside (as this is where the affliction took place) by inhaling smoke from the ashes of medicinal products. 53 manifests the same symptoms as the patient and through this, intuitively determines the cause and treatment. he may use prayer, ritual medication or fulfilment of the forgotten rituals. he also educates parents and siblings on ways to handle the stutterer. s4 communicates with the ancestors, then prepares medication from the dried tongues of certain animals which is rubbed into cuts in the throat. name description cause management outcome attitude of stutterer attitude of society si tsonga (lilele) konkoretsa syllable repetitions heredity (accumulation of coagulated milk in throat.) none medication none not a problem accepting status unaffected. 1 1 ] s2 south sotho kgakgametsa korakoretsa syllable repetitions prolongation visible and audible tension heredity baby left out in first spring rain medication prayer and ritual medication. parental counselling. slow speech easy prolongations problem low self esteem accepting status unaffected. j s3 xhosa thintitha syllable repetitions blocks heredity failure to inform ancestors of imminent child-birth whitchcraft prayer medication rituals parental counselling as with mental disorder slow speech big problem handicapping y accepting status unaffected / s4 zulu amalimi repetitive clicks (syllable repetitions) heredity medication applied to cuts on throat area. slow speech y big problem accepting status affected the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) indigenous healers and stuttering 47 outcome subjects 2, 3 and 4 were agreed that their treatment would not cure the stuttering but would lead to slower, improved speech. therapists in many parts of the world report programs based on slow easy speech as helpful for stutterers (peters & guitar, 1991). subject 2 claimed that "there will be some noticeable change in the child". when asked to demonstrate the change, she showed easy prolongations in place of the effortful blocking she had shown as symptomatic of stuttering. van riper (1973) and others, advocate modifying the symptoms of stuttering to easy prolongations. there is no objective proof that the ih's treatment of stuttering is as successful as reported. it is likely, however, that the treatment could bring relief to the patients, at least partially, through indirect suggestion. indirect suggestion forms part of any stuttering program. a patient seeking therapy, goes with an anticipation and expectation of relief from his stuttering. this strong faith and desperate need are able to bring about a certain amount of relief (van riper, 1973). van riper attributes the success of suggestion to the intermittent, variable, fluctuating nature of stuttering, spontaneous recovery and the fact that temporary fluency is easily established. whether or not any known medication has proved successful in treating stuttering is queried. van riper (1973) cites many experiments done to assess the efficacy of certain drugs but criticizes their methodologies. he concludes that placebos may produce the same effects as drugs, because the therapeutic value of drugs often comes from the patient's faith in his physician. van riper (1973) gives credit for success in therapy, to the therapist's love and concern for the patient, over any other factors. ihs provide "... warm, nurturent, total acceptance of their patients" (hammond-tooke, 1989, p. 147). this, coupled with a belief in their powers of healing and a need to be cured, may be effective in relieving some of the symptoms, e.g. tension, associated with stuttering. counselling parents also'plays a significant part in the therapy provided by speech therapists. the principle of guiding parents in terms of their speech and behaviour towards a stuttering child, is reported by some of the ihs. given the warm empathetic environment that an ih can provide, parents may discuss their anxieties and become open to suggestions regarding the treatment of their children (van riper, 1973). it is not uncommon for parents to report a dramatic cessation in stuttering once they have removed certain pressures from speaking situations (van riper, 1973). strategies, e.g. use of simple language, reducing time pressure, providing a fluency model and not calling attention to the stutter are some of the suggestions that peters and guitar (1991) make reference to in a summary of the parent counselling of theorists such as van riper, bloodstein and luper and mulder. s3 makes similar suggestions, e.g. "... they shouldn't shout at the child, they shouldn't speak fast to the child", and cautions that"... imitating him to tease him creates a big problem ... this shouldn't be done". the success of the ihs treatment may, in some cases, not be attributable to their efforts at all. research has indicated that 50-80% of children who stutter, recover before puberty, without any treatment (peters and guitar, 1991). although they commented that the methods of gathering this information were not entirely reliable, peters and guitar (1991) conclude that spontaneous recovery can occur. attitudes si did not feel that konkoretsa was a problem. the other three concluded that stuttering was a problem which would handicap the individual or cause him to develop a low self esteem. however, only s4 felt that the stutterer would be prevented from attaining a position of status such as a tribe leader. this duty requires proficient, confident speech which is beyond the reach of the stutterer. implications aron (1991) estimates that only a fraction of the more than 3 000 000 people in south africa who require speech therapy, receive it, due to the small number of speech therapists working in this country. as a solution she proposes a community based approach to speech therapy, i.e. training community workers to provide basic therapy and knowledge of when and to whom referrals should be made. ihs are already recognised and consulted by most black south africans. they may, therefore, provide the untapped resource needed to make services accessible to more people with communication disorders, one that includes anawareness of "linguistic and cultural forces that operate" on the individual (shames, 1989, p.74). co-operation between speech therapists and ihs in the treatment of stuttering appears viable, not to undermine or eradicate cultural beliefs, but to share information and establish a system of referral. the beliefs held by ihs have proven to be a fertile and accessible area and more research is indicated in ih's beliefs as to cause and management of other communication disorders, e.g. hearing loss, strokes and cerebral palsy were mentioned by these subjects; the outcome of treatment by an ih from the patient's perspective; interviewing greater numbers of ihs in order to generalize common trends in their beliefs about cause, management and attitudes to stuttering; a comparison of the views of rural and urban ihs. although no marked differences appeared in the views expressed by the subjects in this study, it must be noted that the two rural subjects lived relatively close to an urban area. possibly subjects living in more remote parts of the country might offer different ideas; the efficacy of modern stuttering therapies for black south africans. conclusion at a conference in geneva in 1987, the who resolved to develop traditional medicine in its member states as ihs constitute the most abundant health resource in many countries. in order to utilise and maximise this existing resource, the who has suggested national research strategies into traditional medicine (akerele, 1987). south africa too is scrutinising its national health policy in order to address primary health needs (aron, 1991). these could be satisfied by community workers familiar with the particular linguistic and cultural background of the community. the ih may be considered ideal die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 r o z a n n e p l a t z k y & j o a n g i r s o n in filling this role. this article is limited in terms o f its scope, i.e. only stuttering w a s investigated and only a small sample w a s used. however, it indicates that communication disorders are recognised and treated by ihs, and m o r e research into this field is necessary. further research m a y strengthen the conclusion that co-operation b e t w e e n ihs and speech therapists is possible as "... k n o w l e d g e and sympathetic understanding ... combine in o n g o i n g dialogue, with the interests o f the patient as the overriding concern" (hammond-iboke, 1989, p.155). a c k n o w l e d g m e n t s the writers w i s h to thank the following: 1. t h e a f r i c a n n a t i o n a l h e a l e r s a s s o c i a t i o n for assistance w i t h i n f o r m a t i o n a n d o b t a i n i n g subjects. 2. m r r a k a l u t a t h e interpreter. 3. t h e s u b j e c t s w h o p a r t i c i p a t e d w i l l i n g l y i n the r e s e a r c h a n d s h a r e d their k n o w l e d g e generously. r e f e r e n c e s -african national healers association. unpublished paper on research issues and the anha. akerele, o. (1987). the best of both worlds: bringing traditional medicine up to date. soc. sci. med., vol. 24, no. 2, 177-181. ammons, r. & johnson, w.(1944). studies in the psychology of stuttering. jsd, vol. 9, 39-393. aron, m. l. (1966). the nature and incidence of stuttering among a bantu group of school going children. jshd, vol. 21, no. 2, 116-128. aron, m. l. (1991). perspectives. south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 38, 3-11. babbie, c. r. (1973). survey research methods, california: wadsworth pub. co. bloodstein, o. (1987). a handbook on stuttering, chicago: national easter seal society. fenalson, a. f. (1952). essentials in interviewing, new york: harper & brothers. freeman, m. (1992). lecture on the traditional healer at witwatersrand medical school conference, may 20. forcese, d. p. & richer, s. (1973). social research methods, new jersey: prentice hall, inc. hammond-tooke, w. d. (1974). the bantu-speaking peoples of southern africa, (2nd ed.) london: routledge and kegan paul. hammond-tooke, w. d. (1989). rituals and medicines, johannesburg: a.d. donker. holdstock, t. l. (1979). indigenous healing in s.a.: a neglected potential. south african journal of psychology, vol. 9,118124. johnson, w. (1944). the indians have no word for it: stuttering in children. quarterly journal of speech, vol. 30, 330-337. kahn, r. l. and cannel, c. f. (1957). dynamics of interviewing, new york: john wiley and sons. krober, i. (1990). indigenous healers in a future mental health system: a case for co-operation. psychology in society, vol. 4, p.47-62. leith, w. & mims, h. (1975). cultural influences in the development and treatment of stuttering. jshd, 459-466. leith, w.r. (1986). treating the stutterer with atypical cultural influences. in the atypical stutterer, st. louis k. o. (ed.), orlando, florida: academic press. motlana, n. (1992). lecture on the traditional healer at the witwatersrand medical school conference may 20. mzinyathi, m. (undated). mental health care in s.a.: a personal psychological perspective, 143-145. oppenheim, a. h. (1966). questionnaire design and attitude measurement, london: heinemann educational books ltd. peters, t. j. & guitar, b. (1991). stuttering: an integrated approach to its nature and treatment. baltimore: williams & wilkins. shames, g. h. (1989). stuttering: an rfp for a cultural perspective. jfd, vol. 14, 67-77. smith, t. (1991). communicating quality: professional standards for speech and language therapists. britain: the college of speech and language therapists. snidecor, j. c. (1947). why the indian does not stutter. quarterly journal of speech. van riper, c. (1971). the nature of stuttering, new jersey: prentice hall, inc. van riper, c. (1973). the treatment of stuttering, new jersey: prentice hall, inc. wingate, μ. e. (1972). the evaluation and stuttering: ii environmental stress and critical appraisal of speech. in an analysis of stuttering: selected readings, l. l. emerick & c. c. η am re (eds.), illinois: interstate publishers and printers. young, p. v. (1966). scientific social surveys and research, 4th edition, new jersey: prentice hall inc. j appendix interview f o r m a t a) biographical information 1) n a m e , ethnic group. 2) h o w did you become an ih? 3) did you receive training? 4) for h o w m a n y years have you b e e n practicing as an ih? b) stuttering 1) tell m e about the w o r k you do. 2) have you ever b e e n consulted b y a patient with a speech problem? tell m e about this case. 3) do y o u k n o w what stuttering is? 4) w h a t w o r d do you use for stuttering? 5) have you ever treated a stutterer? cause m a n a g e m e n t o u t c o m e / 6) does your treatment differ if the patient is male or female? 7) would a stutterer's status b e affected because of the stuttering, e.g. could he be a tribe leader? 8) h o w w o u l d y o u rate stuttering: as a big, small or no problem? 9) do you have any questions that you would like to ask m e ? ^ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) d i e w a a r d e van v o o r a f o p l e i d i n g d e u r d i e o u e r vir p e d o o u d i o m e t r i e by drieen v i e r j a r i g e kleuters. merisa lodewyckx b. ( l o g ) (pretoria) departement spraakwetenskap, spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming die verkryging van akkurate, betroubare inligting aangaande die gehoorvermoe van jong kinders is van besondere belang vir die oudioloog, maar word dikwels bemoeilik aangesien die kleuter nie goeie samewerking bied in die vreemde pedo-oudiometriese situasie nie. die oplossing hiervoor is grootliks gelee in die opleiding van die kind voor formele gehoortoetsing en meer spesifiek opleiding tuis deur die ouer. die doel van hierdie studie was om 'n voorbereidende program op te stel wat die ouer tuis met die kind kan uitvoer, voordat formele toetsing plaasvind. die waarde en effek van hierdie program in die pedo-oudiometriese situasie is geevalueer by 'n groep drieen vierjarige kleuters. resultate dui daarop dat laer en meer akkurate drempels, 'n korter toetstydperk, verminderde angs en beter samewerking verkry is by die kleuters wat vooraf opleiding ontvang het. die opleidingsprogram kan dus in die kliniese praktyk benut word as voorafopleiding vir pedo-oudiometrie. summary it is of great importance to the audiologist to obtain accurate, reliable information concerning the hearing sensitivity of the young child but this is often hampered by the infant's poor co-operation in the strange audiometric situation. the solution lies in the training of the child before formal hearing evaluation and more specifically in training by the parent at home. the aim of this study was to compile a training programme which the parent carries out at home, before formal testing of the child. the value and effect of this programme in pediatirc audiometry was evaluated in a group of three and four year old children. results showed that lower and more accurate thresholds, a short test time, reduced anxiety and better co-operation was obtained in infants that had received previous training. in clinical practice, the programme can be used for training of the infant before audiometry. een van die grootste probleme vir elke pediatriese oudioloog is die verkryging van definitiewe en betroubare oudiometriese inligting by die jong kind. inligting van hierdie aard is noodsaaklik vir die identifikasie van afwykings, vir seleksie van 'n toepaslike gehoorapparaat en die daarstelling van toepaslike opleidingsprogramme vir die indiwiduele kind. akkurate meting van gehoor by die volwassene is gebaseer op sy omvangryke ouditiewe ervarings en veral sy akkurate subjektiewe evaluasie van ouditiewe sensasie, asook sy vermoe om verbale instruksies te gehoorsaam. sodanige meting is nie by die jong kind moontlik nie. 3 dit het dus nodig geword om alternatiewe prosedures te ontwikkel wat geldige en betroubare meting van gehoorsensitiwiteit by die kind moontlik maak. operante kondisionering is een van die tegnieke wat groot toepassingsmoontlikhede vir kinderoudiometrie inhou. die prosedure van speloudiometrie is gebaseer op die beginsels the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) voorafopleiding en pedo-oudiometrie 115 van kondisionering en word die algemeenste gebruik in die verkryging van 'n suiwertoonoudiogram by die jong kind. in speloudiometrie vind kondisionering van 'n aangeleerde respons op 'n ouditiewe sein deur middel van die genotsbeginsel (spelaksie) plaas. aangesien speloudiometrie uiteraard 'n kondisioneringsproses is, staan die oudioloog voor die moeilike taak om die kind te kondisioneer om 'n stimulus (ouditief) te assosieer met 'n spesifieke respons (motoriese taak). die kondisioneringstaak word verder bemoeilik deurdat die kind opgelei moet word om betroubare response te lewer op suiwertone wat, soos dit uit die literatuur ook blyk, relatief onbekend en dikwels betekenisloos vir 'n jong kind is. verdere probleme ontstaan as gevolg daarvan dat gehoortoetsing plaasvind in 'n omgewing wat vir die kleuter onbekend is en dit wek dikwels angs en onsekerheid. verder, moet angstige kinders soms opgelei word om die oorfone te aanvaar, aangesien dit noodsaaklik is om elke oor afsonderlik te toets. 1 bogenoemde probleme beklemtoon dat die sukses en waarde van 'n volledige gehoorevaluasie afhanklik is van die betroubare samewerking van die kind. om samewerking te bewerkstellig is 'n kondisioneringsproses nodig voordat die kind se gehoordrempels formeel bepaal kan word. die waarde van 'n opleidingsperiode is deur verskeie outeurs beklemtoon. lowell en andere 4 beskou die voorafopleidingsperiode as die grondslag van drempelbepaling en beklemtoon dat alvorens onafhanklike en onderskeidende response tydens hierdie periode verkry word, geen poging tot drempelbepaling aangewend moet word nie. lowell gebruik •n visueel waarneembare klankbron, dit wil se die ouditiewe stimulus word gekoppel aan 'n waarneembare bron, in die opleidingsperiode. later word die visuele bron verwyder en die kind reageer slegs op die ouditiewe stimulus. variasie van die ouditiewe stimulus in terme van intensiteit en duur met die oog op respons veralgemening, vind ook in die opleidingsperiode plaas.4 o'neill en andere 6 gebruik hierteenoor 'n periode van semigestruktureerde spel voordat drempels bepaal word. die doel van hulle spelperiode is nie, soos die van lowell,4 om onderskeidende response te verkry nie, maar om die beste toetstegniek vir 'n bepaalde kind te selekteer. opleidingsperiodes tydens gehoortoetsing is dikwels tydrowend en die waarde daarvan kan in 'n mate bevraagteken word. die oudioloog is steeds vir die kind 'n vreemdeling en die toetssituasie bly onbekend en bedreigend. dus kan dit nooit met sekerheid aangeneem word dat die kondisioneringsproses bevredigend plaasgevind het nie. miller en polisar5 beklemtoon die feit dat die oudioloog die ouer(s) kan benut om die kind op te lei om konsekwent te reageer op ouditiewe stimuli. hulle is daarvan oortuig dat 'n intelligente, goed aangepaste en tegemoetkomende ouer uitstekende en waardevolle hulp kan bied deur tuis met die kind te oefen. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 116 merisa lodewyckx sodoende word die kind deur die ouer opgelei om die tipe respons, wat in die oudiometriese situasie verwag word, korrek uit te voer. sulke aanvullende oefening en kondisioneringstegnieke tuis kan volgens miller en polisar,5 die aantal besoeke aan die oudioloog noemenswaardig verminder. 'n moontlike oplossing vir 'n groot aantal van die genoemde probleme by gehoortoetsing van voorskoolse kleuters, is gelee in die benutting van die ouer(s) om die kind tuis reeds op te lei en te kondisioneer, voor formele gehoortoetsing plaas vind. dit is dan die doel van hierdie studie om (1) 'n opleidingsprogram op te stel wat aan ouers beskikbaar gestel word, voordat hul kind se gehoor formeel deur 'n oudioloog getoets word en om (2) die waarde van hierdie opleidingsprogram in pedo-oudiometrie te evalueer. eksperiment h i p o t e s e e n d o e l s t e l l i n g s die hipotese is gestel dat kleuters tussen drieen vierjarige ouderdom, wat 'n opleidingsprogram tuis ontvang het, beter sal reageer in 'n pedo-oudiometriese suiwertoon toetssituasie, teenoor soortgelyke kleuters wat nie vooraf opgelei is nie. om hierdie hipotese te bewys, is die volgende doelstellings nagestreef: 1. 'n opleidingsprogram is opgestel wat die ouer tuis met die kind uitvoer om hom/haar voor te berei op formele drempelbepaling. die doel van hierdie program was: (a) om die kind emosioneel voor te berei en te motiveer vir gehoortoetsing, sodat die angs en onsekerheid wat die vreemde omgewing, oudioloog en apparaat dikwels tydens gehoortoetsing by kleuters wek verminder sou word. ( b ) o m die kind, deur middel van eenvoudige, bekende "geraasmakers" op te lei om spesifieke, betroubare response te lewer op klankaanbieding, met ander woorde, om opleiding van stimulus-respons-samehang te verskaf. (c) om die kind op te lei om aanvanklik te reageer op 'n visueel waarneembare klankbron, wat later verwyder word, sodat die kind dan slegs op die ouditiewe sein moet reageer, met ander woorde operante kondisionering word bewerkstellig... / ( d ) o m stimulusveralgemening en -diskriminasie by clie kind te bewerkstellig deurdat die ouditiewe stimuli gevarieer word in terme van intensiteit, toonhoogte, duur en rigting. 2. die waarde van die opleidingsprogram in pedo-oudiometrie is ondersoek. 3. die program is verder geevalueer op grond van 'n bespreking met die ouer, asook 'n kort vraelys' wat deur die ouer voltooi is na uitvoering van die program. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) voorafopleiding en pedo-oudiometrie 117 proefpersone twintig kleuters is gebruik as proefpersone en vir die doel van hierdie eksperiment het elke kind voldoen aan die volgende kriteria: — die kind was in die ouderdomsgroep drie tot vier jaar. — die kind was afrikaanssprekend en van 'n middel sosioekonomiese klas. — die kind het geen persoonlikheids-, fisiese of verstandelike afwykings gehad nie. — die kind het geen vorige ondervinding van gehoortoetsing gehad nie. — die kind het geen oorinfeksies, mangelontsteking, verkoue of griep onderlede gehad tydens toetsing nie. metode 'n voorbereidende opleidingsprogram wat die ouer tuis oor 'n periode van drie dae met die kind uitvoer, is opgestel. die doel en inhoud van hierdie program word kortliks bespreek: die program word oor 'n tydperk van drie dae uitgevoer. spesifieke instruksies en take is onder die opskrifte, dag 1; dag 2; en dag 3, uiteengesit. d a g 1: doelstellings: emosionele voorbereiding van die kind op die oudiometriese situasie, asook keuse van 'n toepaslike responsmedium vir die individuele kind. inhoud: die algemene prosedure wat tydens formele gehoortoetsing verwag kan word, word deur die ouer uitgebeeld in die vorm van 'n storie om die kind voor te berei op die vreemde oudiometriese situasie. die ouer moet ook op dag 1 'n spelaksie/respons kies wat telkens in die program gebruik sal word en waarvan die kind hou. ('n lys van moontlikhede word verskaf.) d a g 2 : doelstellings: motivering, stimulus-responssamehang en stimulusveralgemening. 'n bekende, visueel-waarneembare klankbron wat op bo-drempel vlakke aangebied word, word gebruik om die situasie betekenisvoller en interessanter te maak, sodat die kind meer gemotiveerd is om op die ouditiewe stimuli te reageer. stimulusrespons-samehang word bewerkstellig deurdat die kind 'n definitiewe assosiasie vorm tussen verlangde respons en ouditiewe stimulus. stimulusveralgemening word bewerkstellig deur variasie van stimuli in terme van duur en toonhoogte. inhoud: die ouer lei die kind op om die korrekte respons (waarop besluit tydens dag 1), uit te voer, elke keer as 'n klank aangebied word. vir hierdie doel word daar van 'n verskeidenheid klankbronne gebruik gemaak wat visueel waarneembaar is (die ouer staan voor die die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 118 merisa lodewyckx kind by klankaanbieding). 'n bekende klank word dus op bodrempelvlakke aangebied en dit word gevarieer in terme van duur en toonhoogte. d a g 3 : doelstellings: stimulusveralgemening, stimulusdiskriminasie en versterking. stimulusveralgemening vind plaas aangesien die stimuli weereens gevarieer het in terme van toonhoogte en nou ook in terme van lokalisasie (rigting) en intensiteit (luidheid). stimulusdiskriminasie word bewerkstellig aangesien daar slegs op die ouditiewe sein gereageer moes word, omdat die visuele bron verwyder is. versterking is beoog deurdat sosiale sowel as konkrete beloning verskaf word. inhoud: dieselfde klankbronne word op dag 3 gebruik, maar die verskil is nou dat die klankbron nie meer visueel waarneembaar is nie. stimuli word nou ook gevarieer in terme van intensiteit. die proefpersone is op 'n toevallige wyse verdeel in twee groepe van tien kinders elk. die kontrolegroep, bekend as groep a is onderwerp aan prosedure x en die eksperimentele groep, bekend as groep b, aan prosedure y. prosedure x behels: — 'n suiwertoondrempelbepaling deur middel van speloudiometrie — 'n tussenpose van drie dae — 'n tweede suiwertoongehoortoets deur middel van speloudiometrie prosedure y behels: — 'n suiwertoondrempelbepaling deur middel van speloudiometrie — 'n tussenpose van drie dae waartydens die kind tuis opleiding ontvang deur middel van die opleidingsprogram — 'n tweede suiwertoongehoortoets deur middel van speloudiometrie. die prosedure wat gevolg is by elke suiwertoondrempelbepaling by beide groepe, was kortliks as volg: — die oudioloog het die kind kortliks deur middel van die zenith neometer opgelei om hout blokkies in 'n kartondoos te gooi by aanbieding van 'n ouditiewe stimulus. — luggeleidingsdrempels is bepaal by 1 000 hz; 500 hz; 250 hz, 2 000 hz en 4 000 hz en 'n dalende-stygende metode van drempelbepaling is gebruik. — sosiale versterking tydens drempelbepaling is gebruik om korrekte response te versterk en na afloop van drempelbepaling by elke oor is 'n vorm van konkrete beloning gegee. tydens drempelbepaling is die volgende aspekte deur die oudioloog opgeteken: j (a) tydsduur van drempelbepaling] dit is gemeet met 'n stophorlosie. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) voorafopleiding en pedo-oudiometrie 119 (b) die aantal foutiewe response wat die kleuter gelewer het. dit is elke keer aangeteken as die kind 'n respons lewer (blokkie ingooi) sonder dat 'n stimulus aangebied is. nadat suiwertoondrempels verkry is, is die gemiddelde suiwertoondrempel vir die regterasook linkeroor vir elke proefpersoon, bereken. na voltooiing van drempelbepaling is die volgende aspekte subjektief deur die oudioloog geevalueer en op die voorafopgestelde vorms aangeteken. — samewerking van die toetsling in die toetssituasie op 'n vyfpuntskaal. — angstigheid by die moeder op 'n driepuntskaal. — angs en onsekerheid by die toetsling op 'n driepunt-skaal. — aanvaarding van die oorfone deur die toetsling op 'n vierpuntskaal. slegs by die eksperimentele groep, waar 'n opleidingsprogram uitgevoer is, het die moeder 'n kort vraelys voltooi waarin sy die program evalueer. elke moeder is ook die geleentheid gegun om terugvoering aangaande die program aan die oudioloog te gee. resultate die resultate verkry by die tweede gehoortoets is vergelyk met die van die eerste toetsing om vas te stel of die opleidingsprogram die resultate van die tweede toetsing bei'nvloed het. die kontrolegroep is ook twee keer getoets ten einde te bepaal of, indien die tweede toetsing beter resultate gelewer het, dit aan oefening of aan die opleidingsprogram toegeskryf kan word. die twee groepe word dus vergelyk om te bepaal wat die invloed/effek van die opleidingsprogram is. vergelyking van gemiddelde suiwertoondrempels: om vas te stel of enige verbetering (verkryging van laer drempels) of verswakking (verkryging van hoer drempels) in gemiddelde suiwertoondrempels vanaf die eerste na die tweede toetsing plaasgevind het, is die volgende berekening by beide groepe gedoen: gemiddelde suiwertoon drempel by toets 1 minus gemiddelde suiwertoon drempel by toets 2 (sien tabel i). tabel i dui dat die gemiddelde verbetering in drempelwaarde by die kontrolegroep 0,01 db was wat nie statisties betekenisvol is nie, terwyl die verbetering by die eksperimentele groep 4,18 db was, wat statisties betekenisvol is op die 1% peil. die verbetering in gemiddelde drempelwaarde van die eksperimentele groep, vergeleke met die van die kontrolegroep is 4,18 0,01 = 4,17 db, wat statisties betekenisvol is op die 5% peil. hierdie waarde van 4,17 db, verteenwoordig dus die verbetering in drempelwaarde wat toegeskryf kan word aan die effek van die opleidingsprogram. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 120 merisa lodewyckx t a b e l i : verandering in gemiddelde suiwertoondrempels vir beide ore, vanaf die eerste na tweede toetsing d.w.s. gemiddelde suiwertoondrempel by toets 1 — gemiddelde suiwertoondrempel by toets 2 proefpersone groep a: groep b: 1 4 , 1 db 0 db 2 0 , 8 db -6,65 db 3 4 , 2 db 2,45 db 4 2,5 db 5 db 5 2 , 5 db 5,85 db 6 3,3 db 5,9 db 7 7,5 db 5,05 db 8 2,55 db 7,5 db 9 1 , 7 db 7,5 db 10 -2,45 db 9,2 db gemiddelde 0,01 db 4,18 db standaard-afwyking 3,607 4,38 standaard-fout 1,202 1,46 vefgelyking van tydsduur benodig vir drempelbepaling: tabel ii dui dat by elke proefpersoon in beide groepe die tydsduur benodig vir drempelbepaling by die eerste sowel as tweede gehoortoets opgeteken is en die verskil tussen die twee tydswaardes bereken is. by die kontrolegroep is die verskille tussen waardes verkry in die eerste en tweede toetsing betekenisvol op die 1% peil terwyl die verskille by die eksperimentele groep betekenisvol is op die 0,1% peil (meer betekenisvol). dit blyk duidelik uit tabel ii dat by beide groepe 'n korter tydsduur benodig was om drempels te bepaal by die tweede as by die eerste toetsing. die gemiddelde verskille in waardes! was vir groep a: 1,069 en vir groep b: 2,02. by toepassing van die t-toets vir onafhanklike steekproewe was gevind dat t = 2,000, wat betekenisvol is op die 5% vlak. alhoewel drempels dus by beide groepe vinniger bepaal kan word by die tweede gehoortoets, dui bogenoemde statistiese berekening dat drempels by groep β noemenswaardig vinniger bepaal kon word the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) voorafopleiding en pedo-oudiometrie 121 oh w ο ps ο oo 15 o, "u o, ε > a s "d ο a mass dominated threshold +b-g +b+g +b-g -b+g -b+g ^-b-g -b-g suprathreshold +b-g +b+g -b+g the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) direction of susceptance and conductance acoustic reflexes at high probe frequencies 89 observations was too small to conduct any statistical analysis (fitz-gibbon & morris, 1987) it can be seen that the distribution of reflex patterns conforms to this continuum, with the majority of subjects in group 1 (higher natural frequency, therefore more stiffness dominated) showing the +b-g pattern, while the majority in group 5 (lower natural frequency, therefore more mass dominated) showing the -b-g pattern at both levels. thus as the natural frequency of the system moves from above the probe frequency to below it, one sees the progression of threshold reflex patterns from +b-g to -bg. this suggests that the patterns may occur in each subject, were the probe frequency to be shifted to frequencies above and below the ear's natural frequency. examples of each of the reflex patterns is shown in figure 1. the +b-g pattern was seen most commonly for ears which were stiffness dominated (groups 1 and 2). that is, the probe frequency was below the mean resonant frequency for both of these groups, which were 1200 hz and 1062 hz respectively. this pattern of +b-g is consistent with the findings of feldman & williams (1976) and lutman et al., (1984) who used 660 hz probe frequencies, at which many adult ears have similar transmission properties to those seen for these subjects in groups 1 and 2. this pattern at 660 hz has been related to an increase in stiffness when the reflex is activated (feldman & williams, 1976). the transmission properties for most of the ears demonstrating the +b-g pattern are possibly similar to the ears used in previous investigations at 660 hz, as they showed a fair contribution by negative reactance (stiffness) to overall impedance. this suggests that the effect of the reflex for the +b-g pattern at the 1000 hz probe frequency in the present study may also result from added stiffness when the reflex is activated. although the effects on impedance components were not investigated in the present study, the results do appear to be consistent with theoretical models of the effect of the acoustic reflex presented by lutman & martin (1979) and lutman (1984). j the +b+g pattern was clearly the most common pattern for ears in group 3 where the mean resonant frequency (950 hz) was just below the 1000 hz probe frequency. the +b+g patternl at threshold was also the most common pattern for ears in group 4 in which the transmission properties were slightly above resonance, but not significantly mass dominated. this suggests that where ears demonstrate transmission properties close to resonance, the +b+g pattern is expected. the +b+g pattern shifted to +b-g for several of the observations across groups 1 to 4. a sketch of this phenomenon is shown in figure 2. the direction change observed suggests that the effect of the reflex at suprathreshold intensity levels is to increase the amount of stiffness in the system. as the +b-g pattern at threshold in the present study is more commonly found for ears which are more stiff than those which show the +b+g pattern at threshold, it appears that the direction change to the +b-g pattern at suprathreshold levels is consistent with this addition of further stiffness at high intensities. these intensity related effects of the reflex are referred to in the theoretical models of the effect of the reflex (lutman & martin, 1979)., the -b+g pattern has not been reported previously. this occurred only in ears which demonstrated transmission properties which were above resonance as shown by the tympanometric shape at 1000 hz (groups 4 and 5). the mean resonant frequencies of these two groups was 707 hz and 650 hz respectively, and this pattern suggests that for ears with low resonant frequencies, the expected reflex pattern for high probe frequencies could be -b+g at threshold. the degree to which this pattern is expected appears to increase as the system becomes more mass dominated, as shown by this pattern being more' common for group 5 than for group 4. the -b+g pattern also showed a suprathreshold direction change (in some ears) to the +b+g pattern. a sketch of this is shown in figure 3. as with other direction changes discussed above, this direction change suggests that at higher intensities the effect of the reflex is to show added negative reactance, so that transmission approximates that usually seen for ears closer to resonance. because the effect of the reflex on reactance and resistance was not investigated directly, the patterns can only be simply related to patterns seen at lower probe frequencies. a more thorough investigation would be useful to relate measured admittance patterns to the effect on reactance and resistance. such an investigation would need to correct probe tip measures to the level of the tympanic membrane as there is a nonlinear relationship between impedance measures made at the probe tip and those corrected to the lateral surface of the tympanic membrane (margolis, reflex e x a m p l e s pattern s u s c e p t a n c e (b) c o n d u c t a n c e ( g ) + b g 0.09 0 2 ε ε -0.10 + b g ! 0 2 ε ε + b + g ^ ^ ^ ^ 0.12 0 ε ε 0.14 + b + g a ε 0 ε ε b + g -0.09 0 £ ε ε 0.11 b + g 1 0 £ ε ε b g -0.12 c 8 ε ε -0.13 b g c 8 ε ε figure 1. sketch of the reflex patterns obtained at 1000 hz probe frequency s u s c ep ta n c e ( β ) 0 c i § 0.16 1.09 α j^2.09 κ s u s c ep ta n c e ( β ) 0 c i § c o n d u c ta n c e ( g ) m m ho ο 0.72 α . 1.18 α 0.22 y v c o n d u c ta n c e ( g ) m m ho ο v -0.51 -087 c o n d u c ta n c e ( g ) 94 98 102 106 110 d b h l figure 2. sketch to show an example of the direction change in conductance (+b+g; +b-g) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 90 louise reynolds & lucille ρ morton 1981). such an analysis was beyond the scope of the present investigation which was simply to document reflex patterns at higher probe frequencies, recorded in a similar fashion to conventional clinical measures. the -b-g pattern was recorded only for ears which were clearly mass dominated at the 1000 hz probe frequency (group 5). this was interesting in that this pattern is the expected pattern also for ears which are clearly stiffness dominated, as shown by previous studies where the 220 hz probe frequency was extensively investigated (for example, feldman & williams, 1976; lutman et al., 1984). none of these patterns demonstrated the direction change at suprathreshold intensity levels found with the other reflex patterns. this is interesting in that there is less documented evidence of the effect of the reflex for mass dominated systems (lutman & martin, 1979) and it is possible that clearly mass dominated systems yield susceptance and conductance reflex patterns which do not reflect the added stiffness seen for ears with more stiffness in the baseline condition. as mentioned above, such conclusions cannot be drawn from the present investigation, but are discussed in more depth by reynolds (1993). in addition, this aspect of the effect of the acoustic reflex requires further investigation. conclusion thus it appears from the results of this study that the direction of reflex patterns is determined by the natural frequency of the system in relation to the probe frequency. at suprathreshold levels the effect of the increase in intensity is to increase the stiffness, which for some ears results in a change in reflex pattern. the changed pattern is one which is characteristic of systems with a slightly higher natural frequency, i.e., at suprathreshold levels normal systems may behave like more stiffness dominated systems but not like more mass dominated systems. the results also suggest that the continuum of reflex patterns may be seen in all normal systems provided that one has a sufficient range of probe frequencies. while further investigation of the effect of the reflex on underlying impedance components, particularly for mass dominated systems, is indicated these results do provide some guidelines to the clinical audiologist. reflex patterns need to be interpreted in relation to the ffl 0 . 0 8 0 . 1 1 0 . 1 4 lli ο ζ 2 α λ α . ω ο co c ί ε "v" ν d co 0 . 1 0 0 . 1 4 a 0 . 1 2 0 . 1 4 0 . 1 5 0 . 1 6 λ 0 . 1 6 lu ο ζ η α λ, λ α α ι -ο d α ζ ο ο £ ε ε 9 4 9 8 1 0 2 1 0 6 1 1 0 d b h l figure 3. sketch to show an example of the direction change in susceptance (-b+g; +b+g) natural frequency of the system and the probe frequency. thus high probe frequency reflex patterns cannot be interpreted with confidence without and indication of the natural frequency of the system. furthermore it would appear that the normal suprathreshold patterns can be explained by an increased stiffness occurring along a continuum, and that a departure from this may be abnormal. however, reflex patterns in abnormal systems need to be examined. this study formed part of a masters dissertation submitted to the faculty of medicine, university of cape town. references american national standards institute. (1981). specifications for audiometers (ansi s.26). new york, ansi. american national standards institute. (1987). specifications for instruments to measure aural acoustic impedance and admittance (ansi s3.39). new york, ansi. bennett, m.j. & weatherby, l.a. (1979). multiple probe frequency acoustic reflex measurements. scandinavian audiology 8: 233-239. berlin, c.i. & cullen, j.k. (1980). the physical basis of impedance measurement. in jerger, j. & northern, j (eds) clinical impedance audiometry (2nd ed), american electromedics corp. block, m.g. & wiley, t.l. (1986). acoustic-reflex growth for multitone complexes. journal of speech and hearing research 29: 92-98. creten, w.l., van de heyning, rh. & van camp, k.j. (1985). immittance audiometry: normative data at 220 and 660 hz. scandinavian audiology 14: 115-121. feldman, a.s. & williams, p.s. (1976). tympanometric measurement of the transmission characteristics of the ear with and without the acoustic reflex. scandinavian audiology 5: 43-47. fitz-gibbon, c.t. & morris, l. (1987). how to analyze data. newbury park, sage. funasaka, s. & kumakawa, k. (1988). tympanometry using a sweepfrequency probe tone and its clinical evaluation. audiology 27: 99-1008. grason-stadler. (1989) gsi33, version 2 middle-ear analyzer instruction manual 1733-0121, rev. 2, grason-stadler, inc. greenfield, d.g., wiley, t.l. & block, m.g. (1985). acoustic reflex dynamics and the loudness discomfort level. journal of speech and hearing disorders 50, 14-20. | hall, j.w. (1979). effects of age and sex on static compliance. archives of otolaryngology 105: 153-156. ! lilly, d.l. & shanks, j.e. (1981). acoustic immittance of an enclosed volume of air. in popelka, g.r. (ed) hearing assessment with the acoustic reflex grune and stratton. lutman, m.e. (1984). phasor admittance measurements of the middle ear i: theoretical aspects. scandinavian audiology 13: 253-264. ί lutman, m.e. & martin, a.m. (1977). the response of the acoustic reflex as a function of the intensity and temporal characteristics of pulsed stimuli. journal of sound and vibration 54(3) 345-360. lutman, m.e. & martin, a.m. (1979). development of an electracoustic analogue model of the middle ear and acoustic reflex. journal of sound and vibration 64 (1) 133157. lutman, m.e., mckenzie, h. & swan, r.c. (1984). phasor admittance of the middle ear ii: n o r m a l phasor tympanograms and acoustic reflexes. scandinavian audiology 13: 265-274 ' / margolis, r.h. (1981). fundamentals of acoustic immittance. in popelka, g.r. (ed) hearing assessment with the acoustic reflex. grune and stratton. / margolis, r.h., van camp, k.j., wilson, r.h. & creten, w.l. (1985). multifrequency tympanometry in normal ears. audiology 24: 44-53. moller, a.r. (1961). network model of the middle ear. journal of the acoustical society of america 33(2), 168=176. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) d i r e c t i o n of s u s c e p t a n c e a n d c o n d u c t a n c e a c o u s t i c reflexes at h i g h p r o b e f r e q u e n c i e s 91 reynolds, l. (1991). two component acoustic reflex measures across a range of probe frequencies. presented at the south african speech, language and hearing association national audioly conference, durban, south africa, 16-18 october. reynolds, l. (1993). two component acoustic reflex measures as function of probe frequency. unpublished master dissertation, university of cape town. shanks, j.e., lilly, d.j., margolis, r.h., wiley, t.l. & wilson, r.h. (1988). tympanometry. journal of speech and hearing disorders 53 (4) 354-377. sprague, b.h., wiley, t.l. & block, m.g. (1981). dynamics of acoustic reflex growth. audiology 20: 15-40. van camp, k.j. & creten, w.l. (1976). principles of acoustic impedance and admittance. in feldman, a.s. and wilbur, l.a. (eds) acoustic impedance and admittance the measurement of middle ear function. williams & wilkins. vanhuyse, v.j., creten, w.l. & van camp, k.j. (1975). on the w-notching of tympanograms. scandinavian audiology 4: 45-50. wiley, t.l. & block, m.g. (1979). static acoustic-immittance measurements. journal of speech and hearing research 22: 677-696, 1979. wilson, r.h., & mcbride, l.m. (1978). threshold and growth of the acoustic reflex. journal of the acoustical society of america 63 (1) 147-164. wilson, r.h., s h a n k s , j.e., & k a p l a n , s.k. (1984). tympanometric changes at 226 hz and 678 hz across 10 trials for two directions of ear canal pressure change. journal of speech and hearing research 27:257-266. yantis, pa. (1985). puretone air-conduction testing. in katz, j (ed) handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed) williams and wilkins. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the enigma of fluency: a single case study tessa goldsmith ba (sp & η therapy) (witwatersrand) denise anderson ba (sp & η therapy) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract the article deals with the relapse and maintenance of fluency skills in an adult stutterer following a "fluency-based" treatment programme. recordings of speech were made at relevant intervals throughout an eight month period. immediately after intensive treatment, fluency was established and rate and attitudes normalized. the deterioration of these behaviours and their re-instatement is described. opsomming die artikel handel oor die insinking en behoud van vlotheidsvaardighede in 'n volwasse hakkelaar na 'n "vlotheid-gebaseerde" terapie program. spraakopnames is met gereelde tussenposes gedurende 'n agt maande periode gedoen. spraakvlotheid is kort na die intensiewe behandelingsperiode bereik, terwyl spraakspoed and houdings genormaliseer is. die agteruitgang van bogenoemde gedragspatrone en die herinstelling daarvan word beskryf. one of the most perplexing aspects of the stuttering problem reflected by clinical experience and in the literature, is the relative ease with which most stutterers can become temporarily fluent. the novel conditions identified as inducing fluency in the speech of stutterers have been'explored, among others by bloodstein (1950), wingate (1969) and andrews, howie, dosza and/guitar (1982). several explanations have been proposed for the reduction in stuttering. bloodstein (1950) suggested that stronger or unusual stimulation could account for the change while van riper (1973) cited the distraction phenomenon as being responsible. perkins, rudas, johnson, michael and curlee (1974) hypothesized that fluency inducing conditions facilitate among other features, slowing of transitions, reduced grammatical complexity and decreased frequency of voice onset co-ordination. wingate (1969) concluded from his review of the literature that fluency is induced as a result of an alteration in the manner of vocalization. despite the simplicity of achieving immediate fluency, its permance remains elusive for the stutterer. remediation is a controversial issue as highlighted relatively recently by gregory (1979) in his description of the "speak-more-fluently" as opposed to the "stutter-more-fluently"paradigm. as regards the former, miller (1981) states that all fluency-based programmes have in common, decreased speech rate, fewer stress contrasts and continuous breath flow, which permit the stutterer more time to co-ordinate his systems of respiration, articulation and phonation for speech production. proponents of this approach to remediation are perkins et al. (1974), die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 © sasha 1984 schwartz (1976), shames and florance (1980), webster (1980) and goldberg (1983), among others. the establishment of fluency restricted to clinic situations is meaningless, unless satisfactory schedules for its transfer and maintenance are executed. according to boberg, howie and woods (1979) accounts of successful stuttering therapy are frequently reported before long term outcome is evaluated. the integration of fluency is compounded by the inevitability of the relapse phenomenon. although it has been alluded to frequently, relapse has seldom been investigated experimentally, particularly in relation to the subsequent reinstatement of the fluent response. documented treatment failure may facilitate the recognition of significant features relating to the relapse and maintenance of fluency. it is with this aim in mind that the present single case study is presented. description of subject the subject was a 19 year old male university student who had stuttered since early childhood. no family history of stuttering was reported. the subject stated that he was not initially concerned about his stuttering. he had received intermittent speech therapy for about ten years and occasionally, brief remissions had occurred. the most recent intervention included efforts at direct modification of the severe stuttering symptom and acceptance of this speech pattern. procedure measurement over the eight month intervention period, speech measurements were made at five intervals relevant to the therapeutic r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 tessa goldsmith and denise anderson process i.e. base-line, eight weeks post-onset intervention, at relapse, post-relapse and at follow-up (see figure 1). these measurements involved both the frequency of stuttering (percentage of syllables stuttered % ss) as well as speaking rate (number of syllables spoken per minute spm). only the former appears in figure 1 as this is regarded as the most explicit measure of speech progress. the speech sample consisted of two-hundred words of spontaneous speech and two-hundred words of oral reading. p o s t o n s e t r e l a p s e p o s t f o l l o w u p i n t e r v e n t i o n r e l a p s e t r e a t m e n t (in w e e k s ) figure 1 percentage syllables stuttered of spontaneous speech and reading samples over the 8 month period of therapy. the frequency of stuttering and the speaking rate were calculated according to the formulae suggested by andrews and ingham (1972). a general impression of prosody (including melody, inflection and stress) was formed on the basis of the speech samples. to establish the reliability of these measures, interjudge agreement was determined in terms of percentage concurrence. the authors listened simultaneously to the recordings and made independent judgements of the occurrences of these behaviours. dysfluency was defined as instances of part-word repetitions, whole word repetitions, interjections, revisions, tense pauses and dysrhythmic phonation (adams, sears and ramig, 1982). over the five recording periods, the mean percentage agreement for %ss was 88%, and 93% for spm. perceptions of prosody were discussed until consensus was reached. base line evaluation the subject stuttered on 20,4% of syllables during spontaneous, speech and 18,3% ss during reading. according to wingate's (1976) severity rating guide, he presented as a severe stutterer. his speech was characterised by predominantly tense syllable and sound repetitions, laryngealization, and extreme difficulty initiating phonation. stuttering occurred primarily on vowels, nasals, glides and on the first sound and syllable within words. associated secondary features included loss of eye-contact during the moment of stuttering, fidgeting and touching his body. avoidance and starter techniques, especially interjections, were frequently observed. he spoke at a normal rate of 196 spm (mean rate per minute has been defined as 200 ± 34 syllables, andrews and ingham, 1972) and was monotonous when he stuttered and when he was fluent (see table 1). to determine the optimal fluency inducer to be used as a basis for intervention, the subject's response was assessed under the conditions outlined by andrews et al (1982). fluency was induced by slowing speech rate and prolonging phonation. the subject obtained a low score on erickson's scale of communicative attitudes (1969) which is indicative of a positive attitude towards communication. it is clinically significant, relative to the severity of the stuttering symptom, that he also perceived his stuttering as mild in most speaking situations. this positive communicative attitude belies the fact that the subject reported few social interactions generally. intervention as mentioned above, slowing speech rate and prolonging phonation induced fluency in this subject. he was therefore enrolled in the stutter-free speech programme (shames and florance, 1980) which focuses essentially on these behaviours. he made a commitment to the intensive schedule of therapy and home practice. briefly, the aims of this programme are to establish speech that is free from stuttering and a selfperception that is compatible with this new speaking behaviour. the therapy programme is comprised of five overlapping phases, involving the principles of operant conditioning to shape forward moving speech with continuous phonation and controlled speech rate. the following alterations were made to shames and florance's (1980) original stutter-free speech programme: — delayed auditory feedback was excluded as the subject was able to achieve the target response through imitation of the clinician. , table 1 summary of speech measurements over the 8 month intervention period time of evaluation sample percentage syllables stuttered syllables per minute prosody i base-line evaluation speech reading 20,4 18,3 196 187 monotone monotone 8 weeks intervention speech reading 0 0 170 170 normal normal 10 weeks (relapse) speech reading 8,3 0 200 170 normal , normal 20 weeks (post-relapse) speech reading covert speech recording 1,2 0 6,5 179 164 174 normal normal 'normal 32 weeks (follow-up) speech reading covert speech recording 0,6 0 1,8 180 '175 190 normal normal normal the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the enigma of fluency: a single case study 49 — non-verbal signalling (overt hand movement) was omitted during the self-monitoring phase as it was feared that it would become a distraction. establishment of voluntary control of speech was achieved within a three week period of intensive therapy for an hour a day. the subject moved smoothly through the transfer phase-experiencing stutter-free speech in all pre-planned contract activities. eight weeks post-onset intervention the subject was speaking with 0% syllables stuttered, normal prosodic features and at a rate of 170 syllables per minute (refer to table 1). approximately fifty percent of his speech was monitored and fifty percent unmonitored i.e. coincidental fluency. at this stage, the subject was enrolled into a group of adult stutterers, the aims of which were to provide peer support and the opportunity to practice monitored speech. ten weeks post-onset intervention (relapse) the newly learnt speech pattern was transient since the subject began to experience difficulty initiating phonation. marked laryngeal tension, infrequent tense repetitions, sporadic blocks and hesitations were observed. moreover, the subject was unable to reinstate monitored speech despite considerable effort and motivation. his speaking rate had increased to preintervention levels which obviated the previously established voluntary control. stuttering increased as a result but relative to the base-line evaluation, these instances were reduced in frequency, were less pronounced and involved less physical effort (refer to table 1). it seemed that the subject's success in the maintenance phase led to a false sense of security and reduced attention to monitored speech. this is compatible with the concept of "lucky fluency" (perkins, 1981). in addition, environmental events probably instrumental in maintaining the off-target behaviour included the subject's prolonged illness and mid-year examinations at that time. i / ι / i to reinstate the target response] the subject was encouraged to isolate the circumstances in which relapse occurred and the frequency and consequences of its occurrence. in addition to the strategies recommended by shames and florence (1980), intensive therapy was reintroduced and pre-planned contracts were rescheduled. the subject's awareness of his off-target behaviour was increased in an attempt to prevent further relapse. twenty weeks post-onset intervention (post-relapse) after the re-establishment of stutter-free speech throughout the talking day i.e. at 20 weeks post-onset intervention, a further evaluation was conducted. with the aid of 75% monitored speech and 25% unmonitored speech, the percentage of syllables stuttered was reduced to 1,2 % — within normal limits according to andrews and ingham (1972). the rate of speech was controlled at 179 spm. a comparison of these measures in relation to those conducted previously is contained in table 1. it is interesting to note that despite the deterioration in spontaneous speech, oral reading performance remained unaffected. fluency during oral reading was easier to establish at all phases largely because of the reduced propositionality and effective time planning it offers i (perkins, bell, johnson and stocks, 1979). the recency of the relapse highlighted the need for a more detailed description of the subject's reacquired fluency. ingham and packman (1978), howie, tanner and andrews (1981) and andrews and craig (1982) emphasize that a comprehensive evaluation of fluency requires overt and covert assessments of speech behaviour. in operant conditioning terms, the speech clinician and the clinic become the discriminative stimuli for fluency and thus an overt recording of speech would constitute a biased evaluation of improvement. the inclusion of a covert assessment involved the recording of a telephone conversation unbeknown to the subject. a comparison of the overt and covert recordings (contained in table 1) revealed a discrepancy i.e. more stuttering which was probably respresentative of the subject's speech in non-clinic situations. furthermore, the research of runyan and adams (1978, 1979) and runyan, hames and prosek (1982) has revealed that the speech of "successfully therapeutized" stutterers is perceptibly different from normal speakers i.e. 0% syllables stuttered and normal speaking rate do not alone constitute natural fluency. therefore, a perceptual evaluation of the fluency was conducted which focused on the parameters of rate, fluency, naturalness and prosody (ingham and packman, 1978). a spontaneous speech sample was video-recorded of six males aged between 18 and 25 years. three of these were normal speakers and three were "successfully therapeutized" stutterers, one of whom was the subject of the study. six speakers were chosen to provide a heterogenous matrix from which the identified subject could be objectively evaluated. the recordings were randomly presented to five unsophisticated judges, in the same session, who rated each speaker along rating scales developed for the parameters discussed above (see fig. 2). no case history information was provided so that the judges' ratings would not naturalness scale 1 2 natural unnatural fluency scale 1 2 i i 3 i 4 i severe mild stuttering stuttering fluent exceptionally fluent rate scale 1 2 i i 3 i 4 i extremely slow slow fast exceptionally fast prosody scale 1 2 i ι 3 i 4 i extremely monotonous expressive extremely monotonous expressive f i g u r e 2 r a t i n g s c a l e s f o r p e r c e p t u a l e v a l u a t i o n ( a f t e r i n g h a m a n d p a c k m a n , 1 9 7 8 ) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 tessa goldsmith and denise anderson be contaminated. while the ratings of all the speakers carry clinical and theoretical significance, only the identified subject will be discussed for the purposes of this paper. a mean score was computed for each speaker on each parameter. overall agreement was calculated in terms of percentage of agreement between judges and was found to be 90%. the judges perceived the subject as an unnatural speaker, with mild stuttering, slow speaking rate and monotonous speech (refer to table 2). the presence of these abnormal features aroused concern in view of adams and runyan's (1981) consideration of the subtle signs of fluency which they stress are important precursors to relapse. having detected these behaviours, additional maintenance strategies were introduced focusing on the more subtle areas of natural speech. through the use of audio-visual techniques, the subject's laboured rate, excessive monotone and instances of "microstutterings" (boberg et al. 1979) were modified. table 2 mean ratings of perceptual evaluation at relapse parameter subject ns1 ns2 ns3 sts1 sts2 naturalness 2 1 1 1 1 2 fluency 2 3 2 3 2 3 rate 2 3 3 3 3 2 prosody 2 3 3 3 3 1 ns normal speaker sts "successfully therapeutized" stutterer thirty-two weeks (follow-up) therapy contact was discontinued at this point as the subject was on a three-month vacation. subsequent to his return, i.e. eight months after the commencement of the intervention, a complete follow-up evaluation was conducted. as displayed in tables 1 and 3, the subject maintained fluency with normal rate and prosody in spontaneous speech and reading samples. in contrast to the previous evaluation, the present results of the overt and covert measures were consistent. in addition, the subject was now perceived as a natural speaker with normal fluency, and with normal rate and prosody relative to the matrix of normal speakers in the sample. table 3 mean ratings of perceptual evaluation at follow-up parameter subject ns1 ns2 ns3 sts1 sts2 naturalness 1 1 1 1 1 2 fluency 3 3 3 3 2 2 rate 2 2 2 2 2 2 prosody 2 3 2 3 2 2 ns normal speaker sts "successfully therapeutized" stutterer discussion although fluency was achieved and speech rate and attitudes normalised i.e. the ultimate therapy goals realised, the writers prefer to consider the clinical insights raised by the elusiveness of fluency. it is well documented that the establishment of fluency is a relatively uncomplicated procedure. however, the complexity of the client's integration of this fluency into his total life system together with the psychological adaptation involved, cannot be underestimated. intensive operant conditioning programmes frequently set 0% ss as the target for stutter-free speech (howie et al. 1981; goldberg, 1983). other programmes permit up to 2 and 3% ss as falling within the normal non-fluency range (hanna and owen, 1977). although the subject had achieved these criteria for fluent speech production, he nevertheless experienced a relapse in fluency. varying theoretical viewpoints have been proposed which might have accounted for the deterioration of the established response in this subject. prins (1970) has coined the term "stuttering overkill", which he sees as a by-product of intensive therapy schedules. the subject experienced a change in speech behaviour in three weeks and may not have been fully aware of how the fluency was established. webster (1980), in support of prins, blames the inadequate learning of fluency producing skills for the relapse and van riper (1973) points out that the habitstrength of the stuttering behaviour cannot be ignored. the presence of "lucky fluency" (perkins, 1981) is a common phenomenon experienced by many stutterers in the establishment of fluent speech. in this case, the subject felt over confident which led to a reduction in the intensity of monitored practice. subsequently "microstutterings" (boberg et al. 1979) developed which increased in magnitude and ultimately resulted in overt stuttering behaviour. clinicians who overlook the presence of these "microstutterings" may not have been sufficiently rigorous in the fulfillment of performance criteria and response contingencies and thus they may be a precipitator of relapse (shames, 1981). as illustrated by the subject of this study, lucky fluency is transient and carries with it speech that is not under the direct control of the speaker. collectively these correlates of relapse compound the integration of fluency. the effective management of relapse will determine the maintenance of fluency, which is described as "the perennial weak link in the therapeutic chain" (perkins, 1979, p. 119). i issues that face clinicians regarding relapse encompass its predictability, its prevention, its detection and its management. as researchers are unanimous that relapse is inevitable in adult stutterers, so they agree that clients should be informed of tlie likelihood of its occurrence at the outset of the therapeutic process (boberg et al. 1979; florance and shames, 1980; kamhi, 1982; perkins, 1983). sheehan (1979) suggests that relapses should be clinically induced so that the dread of them will be lessened when they occur. the clinician's ability to detect subtle signs of off-target behaviour will depend on the obtaining of a representative sample of speech behaviour, especially as clinic speech. performance alone is not a valid indication of therapy outcome. it is possible that the extent of this subject's relapse could have been minimized had more refined and detailed speech measures been conducted early on in the therapeutic process. andrews and ingham (1972) and andrews and craig (1982) advocate the use of non-clinic covert speech recordings in single case studies. despite the fact that covert assessment is / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the enigma of fluency: a single case study fraught with ethical problems, the discrepancy between the subject's overt and covert speech performance yielded valuable clinical direction. subtle signs of stuttering have been termed "tenuous fluency" by adams and runyan (1981). manifestations of such behaviour include the presence of monotone and rhythmicity, reduction in speaking rate, reduced vocal intensity and inappropriately placed pauses. at the time of the relapse, the subject of this study evidenced signs of all of the above-mentioned behaviours. in their article of 1981, adams and runyan contend that clients who show these signs of tenuous fluency are "good bets for relapse" (p. 215). this study demonstrates that focus on speech behaviour alone does not guarantee improved speech performance. concentrated work on aspects of psychological change is as relevant as are fluency instating procedures. "maintenance of attitude change has been shown to parallel maintenance of speech improvement" (dalton, 1983, p. 170). failure to confront these changes may give the client reason to return to his previous modes of speech behaviour. it became evident that this subject needed most support after the intensive period of therapy. at this point, the programme had to be directed specifically at his feelings about stuttering, his self concept, ideas about becoming a fluent speaker, and his perception of his role in therapy. these issues, as well as the behaviour change necessary for development of confidence to apply speech process control, occur primarily within the context of the clinical relationship. in recent years, perkins (1981, 1983) has argued strongly for the development and periodic re-evaluation of realistic goals for each client. this has emerged from his consideration of "fluency cost-effectiveness". in clients whose fluent speech is only realised through constant monitoring, the energy expended may not be commensurate with the reward of fluency. thus fluency may become a "tiring chore" for the stutterer. perkins (1983) makes the statement that "the expectation of permanent fluency is an expectation rarely realised" (p. 158). perhaps it is only possible if the patient constantly uses the skills he is taught. in his therapy programme, perkins (1981) thus establishes what he has termed "normal sounding speech" as opposed to "normal speech" behaviour. the distinction lies in the fact that normal speakers are not required to do anything special to achieve fluent speech. ^ carefully structured therapy programmes do not necessarily follow the smooth implementation their descriptions suggest. it is the work on motivation, frustration, impatience and disappointment, tolerance of fluency failure, resistance to transfer and failure to use the newly acquired fluency in previously stressful speaking situations which forms the essence of therapy. without these support skills, "clients will probably learn what to do to remain fluent without acquiring the facility to do it" (perkins, 1983, p. 158). conclusion this description was offered in order to capture the elusiveness of the fluent response in an adult stutterer. that it was eventually achieved is not tantamount to its permanence. ultimately it is the individual who chooses fluency. 51 acknowledgements the authors acknowledge south african inherited disorders association — cleft pals for their financial assistance. references adams, m. r. and runyan, c. m. stuttering and fluency: exclusive events or points on a continuum. journal of fluency disorders, 6, 197-218, 1981. adams, m.r., sears, r.l. and ramig, p. vocal changes in stutterers and non-stutterers during monotoned speech. journal of fluency disorders, 7, 21-35, 1982. andrews, g. and craig, a. stuttering: overt and covert measurement of the speech of treated subjects. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 47, 96-99, 1982. andrews, g. and ingham, r. j. an approach to the evaluation of stuttering therapy. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 15, 296-302, 1972. andrews, g. howie, p. μ., dosza, m. and guitar, β. e. stuttering: speech pattern characteristics under fluency inducing conditions. journal of speech and hearing research, 25, 208-216, 1982. bloodstein, o. a rating scale study of conditions under which stuttering is reduced or absent. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 15, 29-36, 1950. boberg, ε., howie, p. m. and woods, l. maintenance of fluency: a review. journal of fluency disorders, 4,93-116,1979. dalton, p. major issues for the therapist. in approaches to the treatment of stuttering. dalton, p. (ed.). croom-helm, london, 1983. erickson, r. l. assessing communication attitudes among stutterers. journal of speech and hearing research, 12,711-724, 1969. florance, c. and shames, g. stuttering treatment: issues in transfer and maintenance. seminars in speech, language and hearing, 1, 375-388, 1980. goldberg, s. a. the development of fluency through behavioral cognitive stuttering therapy. communicative disorders, 8, 89-107, 1983. gregory, η. h. controversial issues: statement and review of the literature. in controversies about stuttering therapy. gregory, h.h. (ed.). university park press, baltimore, 1979. hanna, r. and owen, n. facilitating transfer and maintenance of fluency in stuttering therapy. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 42, 65-76, 1977. howie, p. μ., tanner, s. and andrews, g. short and long term outcome in an intensive treatment program for adult stutterers. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 46, 104-109, 1981. ingham, r.j. and packman, a.c. perceptual assessment of normalcy of speech following stuttering therapy. journal of speech and hearing research, 21, 63-73, 1978. kamhi, a.g. the problem of relapse in stuttering: some thoughts on what might cause it and how to deal with it. journal of fluency disorders, 7, 459-467, 1982. miller, s. airflow therapy programs: facts and/or fancy. journal of fluency disorders, 7, 187-202, 1982. perkins, w. h. from psychoanalysis to discoordination. in controversies about stuttering therapy. gregory, h.h. (ed.). university park press, baltimore, 1979. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 perkins, w.h. measurement and maintenance of fluency. in maintenance of fluency. boberg, e. (ed.). elsevier, new york, 1981. perkins, w. h. learning from negative outcomes in stuttering therapy: ii. an epiphany of failures. journal of fluency disorders, 8, 155-160, 1983. perkins, w. h., rudas, j., johnson, l., michael, w.b. and curlee, r. f. replacement of stuttering with normal speech: iii clinical effectiveness. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 39, 416-428, 1974. perkins, w. η., bell, j., johnson, l. and stocks, j. phone rate and the effective planning time hypothesis of stuttering. journal of speech and hearing research, 22, 747-755,1979. prins, d. improvement and regression in stutterers following short term intensive therapy. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 35, 123-134, 1970. runyan, c. m. and adams, m.r. perceptual study of the speech of "successfully therapeutized" stutterers. journal of fluency disorders, 3, 25-39, 1978. runyan, c. m. and adams, m. r. unsophisticated judges' perceptual evaluation of the speech of "successfully treated" stutterers. journal of fluency disorders, 4, 29-38, 1979. tessa goldsmith and denise anderson runyan, c.m., hames, p. e. and prosek, r.a. a perceptual comparison between paired stimulus methods of presentation of the fluent utterances of stutterers. journal of fluency disorders, 7, 71-77, 1982. schwartz, m. f. stuttering solved. lippincott, philadelphia, 1976. shames, g. and florence, c. l. stutter-free speech: a goal for therapy. charles e. merrill, ohio, 1980. shames, g. relapse in stuttering. in maintenance of fluency. boberg, e. (ed.). elsevier, new york, 1981. sheehan, j.g. current issues on stuttering and recovery. in controversies about stuttering therapy. gregory, h.h. (ed.). university park press, baltimore, 1979. van riper, c. the treatment of stuttering. prentice-hall inc., new jersey, 1973. webster, r. l. evolution of a target-based behavioral therapy for stuttering. journal of fluency disorders, 5, 303-320, 1980. wingate, μ. e. sound pattern in "artificial" fluency. journal of speech and hearing research, 12, 677-686, 1969. wingate, m.e. stuttering: theory and treatment. irvington publishers inc., new york, 1976. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes papers concerned with research, or critically evaluative theoretical or therapeutic issues dealing with disorders of speech, voice, hearing or language, or on aspects of the processes underlying these. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material that has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. form of manuscript. authors should submit manuscripts including artwork, tabular material and photographs etc. in triplicate (the original and two copies). manuscripts should be double spaced with wide margins and should not exceed 20 pages. each page should be numbered and labelled with the author's name. page j should contain only the article title, name of author/s, highest degree and address or institutional affiliation. page 2 should contain only an abstract (100 words) which should be provided in both english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors.. major headings, where applicable, should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements and references. tables and figures should be prepared on separate sheets (one per table/figure, unless two/more are to appear together). the first author's name should appear on the back of each page in pencil. the heading for tables appears above and for figures below. lettering should be uniform, profesionally done and large enough to be legible after a 50% reduction in printing. line drawings must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper. tables and figures should be numbered in order of appearance (with arabic numerals). the amount of tabular and illustrative material allowed will be at the discretion of the editor (usually not more than 6). references. references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and date e.g. van riper (1971). use all authors' names the first time that the reference is used in the text. thereafter, et al. will suffice. references should be listed alphabetically in double-spacing at the end of the article. for acceptable abbreviations of names of journals, consult the fourth issue of dsh abstracts or the world list of scientific periodicals. the number of references should not exceed 20. note the following examples. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. sharf, g.t. some relationships between measures of early language. j. speech hear. dis., 1972, 37, 67-74. crystal, d. clinical linguistics. disorders of human communications 3. arnold, g.e., winckel, f & wyke, b.d. (eds.). austria: springer-verlag, 1981. proofa. galley proofs will be sent to the author wherever possible. corrections other than typographical errors will be charged to the author. reprints. 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. all manuscripts and correspondence should be addressed to: the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, the south african speech and hearing association. p.o. box 31782, braamfontein 2017, south africa. inligting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer artikels oor navorsing, asook krities evaluerende artikels oor die teoretiese of terapeutiese aspekte van spraak-, stem-, gehoorof taalafwykings, of oor aspekte onderliggend aan hierdie afwykings. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings sal nie materiaal aanvaar wat erens anders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. vorm van die manuskrip. skrywers moet die manuskrip, insluitend tekeninge, tabelle en fotos ens. in triplikaat (die oorspronklike en twee afskrifte( inhandig. die manuskripte moet dubbel-gespasieerd met bree kantlyne getik word en nie meer as 20 bladsye oorskry nie. elke bladsy moet genommer wees en die skrywer se naam dra. op bladsy 1 moet slegs die artikel se titel, naam van die skrywer/s, hoogste graad behaal en adres of naam van betrokke geaffilieerde instansie verskyn. op bladsy 2 moet slegs die opsomming (100 woorde) in afrikaans en engels verskyn. hoofopskrifte, waar van toepassing, moet in die volgorde metode resultate, bespreking, gevolgtrekking, erkenning en verwysings wees. tabelle en figure moet op aparte blaaie voorberei word, (een per tabel/figuur, tensy twee of meer saam moet verskyn). die eerste skrywer se naam moet in potlood op die keersy van elke bladsy verskyn. die opskrif vir tabelle moet bo-aan en vir figure onder-aan verskyn. lettertipes moet eenvormig, professioneel gedoen wees, en groot genoeg, om na 50% verkleining in druk, nog leesbaar te wees. lyntekentnge moet oorspronklik, in swart ink en op goeie kwaliteit wit papier gedoen wees. tabelle en figure moet in volgorde (arabiese syfers) genommer word. die hoeveelheid getabuleerde en geillustreerde material wat toegelaat word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). verwysings. verwysings moet in die teks aangebring word deur die naam van die outeur en die datum te verstrek bv. van riper (1971). gebruik alle outeursname wanneer dit die eerste maal aangehaal word. daarna sal et al. voldoende wees. verwysings moet alfabeties gerangskik word aan die einde van die artikel en in dubbelspasieering getik word. voorbeeld: van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. sharf, g.t. some relationships between measures of early language. j. speech hear. dis., 1972, 37, 67-74. crystal, d. clinical linguistics. disorders of human communications 3. arnold, g.e., winckel, f & wyke, b.d. (eds.). austria: springer-verlag, 1981. vind die lys van aanvaarde afkortinge van name van tydskrifte in dhs abstracts (vierde uitgawe) of the world list of scientific periodicals. die aantal verwysings mag nie 20 oorskry nie. proewe. galeiproewe sal waar moontlik aan die skrywer voorgele word vir proeflees. onkoste van veranderinge (uitgesluit tipografiese foute) sal deur die skrywer gedra moet word. afdrukke. 10 afdrukke sonder buiteblaaie sal gratis aan die skrywer verskaf word. alle manuskripte en korrespondensie moet gerig word aan: die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraaken gehoorheelkunde, posbus 31782. braamfontein 2017, suid-afr1ka. die suid-afrikaanse ydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a clinical rating scale of speech dysfunction in parkinson's disease thompson, alison k., b.a. (sp. & h.th.) (witwatersrand) speech therapy department, general hospital, johannesburg. summary the speech dysfunction of parkinson's disease is complex and individually variable owing to the interaction of muscle rigidity, tremor and disturbance of movement. eight speech dimensions which are characteristically disturbed in parkinson's disease are discussed with reference to available research findings. in order to provide a more detailed description of the speech than could be obtained by clinical notes alone, a speech rating scale has been developed, and is presented in summarized form for clinical use. incidence and progression of the speech dysfunction are considered in addition to the problems of assessment peculiar to the patient with parkinson's disease. opsomming die spraakdisfunksie van parkinson se siekte is kompleks en verskil van persoon tot persoon, as gevolg van die interaksie van spierstyfheid, tremor en versteuring van beweging. agt aspekte van spraak wat kenmerkend versteurd is in parkinson se siekte word bespreek met verwysing na die bevindinge van navorsing. 'n spraakgraderingskaal is ontwikkel om η meer volledige beskrywing van spraak te verkry as wat moontlik is met die gebruik van kliniese aantekeninge alleen en dit word opgesom vir kliniese gebruik. die voorkoms en ontwikkeling van die spraakdisfunksie word in oenskou geneem, asook die probleme by evaluasie wat eie is aan die pasient met parkinson se siekte. parkinson's disease is characterized by a combination of muscle rigidity, tremor and disturbance of movement. this disturbance may take the form of slowness of movement (bradykinesia), paucity of movement (hypokinesia) or difficulty in initiating movement (akinesia). the fact that this triad of symptoms occurs to a variable degree in any given patient means that each patient presents with his own individual pattern of both physical and speech dysfunction. speech disturbance, which is generally present to some degree, results from the inability to co-ordinate and integrate the various elements which comprise the motor-speech act. although laboratory techniques have been used to describe the different speech features of parkinson's disease objectively, these are neither practical nor available for everyday clinical use. furthermore, sarno1 8 notes that a patient's intelligibility rating does not necessarily correlate with the degree of motor impairment observed in the speech musculature or with an acoustic description of the speech production. the task of assessing the patient's speech therefore ultimately falls to the clinician who must use subjective judgements together with those recording techniques which are available. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 alison thompson owing to the complexity and individual variation noted in the speech dysfunction of the parkinson's patient, it was felt that a consideration of each discrete speech dimension would lead to a better understanding of the precise nature and level of the patient's speech functioning than could be obtained from clinical notes alone. as speech performance is one index of the patient's general level of functioning, this information is of assistance in the formulation of decisions for ongoing medical management. research studies have provided data on the sequence of speech deterioration with progression of the disease.1' 5 ' 1 4 the course of the patient's speech deterioration can therefore be determined with the aid of a detailed speech assessment obtained at regular intervals. this article describes a clinical rating scale which has been developed to permit a systematic recording of eight speech dimensions which are characteristically disturbed in the parkinson's disease patient. the range of dysfunction pertaining to each speech dimension is discussed in some detail, thereby providing a base reference for applying the scales which are presented in summarized form for clinical use. whilst the scales cannot claim to be fully comprehensive, they provide a simple and practical means of recording clinical judgements for future reference. incidence and progression of speech dysfunction on subjective evaluation, reduced intensity of speech and monotony are frequently the earliest presenting signs of speech disturbance. laryngeal functioning is generally affected early in the disease process, followed by a deterioration in lingual, and finally labial functioning. canter5 demonstrated that in 17 patients tongue-tip involvement was typically greater than lip involvement. blonsky et al,1 investigated the speech of 100 patients with parkinson's disease using cinefluoroscopic techniques. the following incidence of dysfunction was found, (listed below in order of their presentation in the disease process): hoarseness 90 % vocal tremor 30 % lingual dysfunction 50 % labial dysfunction 30 % dysfluency/stuttering 20 % logemann et al'3 classified the voice and articulation defects of 150 patients by perceptual evaluation. they found that many patients had voice disorders in the presence of normal articulation but the converse was true in only one patient. voice disabilities appear to develop before articulation begins to degenerate. their analysis of the sequence of articulatory degeneration reveals' that deterioration progresses from posterior to anterior tongue positions of articulation. first phonemes affected are / k / and /g/, followed by / s / /z/, then / s / /z/, and then / c / / j/. last to be affected are / t / and / d / . jthe labial phonemes / p / / b / hi and / v / were affected after / k / and /g/, usually about the time that / c / and /'j/ deteriorated. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) rating scale of speech in parkinson's 4 1 v r e d o r t the following incidence of speech dysfunction: ι ney c f 8 q 0, voice disorders fi> (hoarseness, breathiness and roughness) articulation disorders 45 % rate disorders 20% n a s a l i t y , κ i n ? unimpaired speech 10 h rnnciderine that one might predict nasality in parkinson's patients owing ^ hp ditv and hypokinesia of the velum, it is noteworthy that this is not a characteristic feature of the condition. the logemann et a p incidence of if) % may reflect normal distribution factors. the relationship between the nature of speech disturbance and the natient who presents with either a predominance of tremor, rigidity or hradvkinesia is not clear. the relative interaction of these signs in the individual would appear to be responsible for the considerable variation in sneech disturbance noted between patients. the speed of speech which can be either abnormally fast or abnormally slow, provides a good example of this variability in presentation. factors influencing assessment there are several factors to be considered when assessing speech performance of the parkinson's disease patient. medication dopaminergic drugs, if well tolerated by the patient, generally result in improved speech."' 1 7 leanderson et al" obtained emg recordings of patients before and during l-dopa treatment. the untreated patient demonstrated a constant muscular hyperactivity which interfered markedly with articulation. a further finding was a disturbance in reciprocal muscular activation, manifested in a simultaneous contraction of opposing articulatory muscles. the primary action of l-dopa was a reduction in hypokinesia, thereby resulting in both improved speech and facial movement. however, from clinical experience, if the patient experiences dyskinesia as a side effect of dopaminergic drugs, then speech performance may once again deteriorate. the patient should be questioned as to his general fluctuation in functioning during the course of the day. stable control is not necessarily maintained, despite regular medication. the appearance of the on-ott reaction may cause marked fluctuation in speech functioning from hour to hour speech performance can be expected to deteriorate concomi t t a n t with a deterioration in the patient's general physical status. excess or deficiency of saliva in the mouth -has an indirect effect on speech performance. t h e untreated patient, plus those not on anticholinergic medication, have a tendency to drooling and slushy articulation. this is believed to be due to reduced effectiveness and frequency of swallowing rather than any excess in saliva secretion. 1 he die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 alison thompson majority of treated patients are placed on anticholinergic medication with the result that they complain of an abnormally dry mouth. this imparts a certain "thickness" of quality to the articulation. almost without exception, patients with this problem report increased difficulty with articulation as saliva secretion diminishes. in this regard, the normal stress reaction of the patient to examination adds to an already existing problem. emotional behaviour patients may also demonstrate a marked breakdown in performance as a result of stress. anxiety and self-consciousness create physical tension which exacerbates the rigidity, tremor and movement disturbance. the patient's speech frequently deteriorates as soon as he faces the clinician and anticipates that his performance is to be assessed. it is necessary to ensure that the patient is relaxed and adjusted to the environment before attempting any assessment. whilst the degree of breakdown under stress does afford valuable information in its own right, the clinician's primary aim is to obtain a rating of the patient's average functional speech ability. many patients are subject to depression which results in a general lack of spontaneity of speech plus poor motivation to strive for optimal functioning. anti-depressant drug therapy can alleviate the condition considerably, leading to an overall improvement in communicative behaviour. time of assessment factors such as fatigue and dosage schedule of drugs may cause significant fluctuation of performance during the day. this applies especially to therapy with l-dopa where performance (ability) may diminish markedly as the effect of the drug wears off and the next dose falls due. ideally, the patient should be interviewed at the same time of day for each subsequent assessment. this would in part help to control these factors. it is nevertheless essential to note the patient's physical and emotional state at the time of assessment. hearing as onset of parkinson's disease clusters around the middle years it follows that many patients will have some degree of presbyacutic hearing loss. the patient decompensates in his speech performance owing to a breakdown in his auditory monitoring skills. speech dysfunction may consequently be the result of a double pathological process' speech sample and assessment technique the parkinson's disease speech ratingscale is subject to the limitations of all rating scales. qualitative, subjective judgements are required which suggests that some experience with parkinson's disease speech is desirable in order to apply the scales 'most effectively. a rating scale of this nature cannot provide absolute quantitative values nor can it claim to the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ratingscale of speech in parkinson's 43 be equally efficient for the rating of all speech dimensions. some dimensions remain inherently more difficult to evaluate than others. an advantage of this type of scale for recording speech performance is that it focuses attention on each individual speech dimension. it is therefore possible to determine where the greatest changes occur as the disease progresses. generalized statements, such as "disturbed rhythm", provide very little useful information when one is faced with a patient for re-assessment. however, if the clinician has available past ratings of the speed of speech, degree of akinesia, and perseveration of phonemes within a word, he consequently has a meaningful delineation of the manner in which the rhythm was previously judged as being disturbed. recorded ratings are of particular reference value when a patient is assessed periodically over a number of years by several different clinicians. , clinically, it is desirable that assessment of speech in the parkinson s patient should be carried out on a spontaneous speech sample. as a rule, speech intelligibility is markedly more impaired in the spontaneous speech flow than it is in a restricted, formally induced sample. this discrepancy in performance has also been noted in the literature. 5 ' 1 s as soon as one seeks to control the patient's speech output in any way, performance alters. an assessment made on a formally induced sample is therefore a poor reflection of the patient's functional speech ability. a rating scale, as an assessment technique, is well suited for use with a spontaneous speech sample. t h e clinician engages the patient in conversation and waits until the patient has adjusted to the speaking situation before commencing the rating. t h e spontaneous sample also affords the clinician as much time as he requires in order to assess any single dimension. provided that the clinician marks his ratings out of the patient's direct line of vision this assessment technique would seem to have a minimally disruptive effect on the patient and, consequently, his speech performance. a d m i n i s t r a t i o n o f t h e r a t i n g s c a l e t h e patient should be comfortably seated in a quiet room. t h e basic information required from the patient should be obtained before starting the assessment. t h e patient is engaged in conversation on a subject of interest after having been instructed to disregard the clinician's note taking. once the patient has become adjusted to the speaking situation the clinician commences the speech assessment, discontinuing as necessary to maintain the patient in conversation. t h e clinician mentally focuses on each speech dimension placing a cross in the relevant place on the format. recording of ratings should be done out of the patient's direct line of vision. . . ^ t „ a gross clinical rating is required for each speech dimension. a rating ot u denotes no impairment whilst 1, 2 and 3 denote mild, moderate and severe impairment respectively. assess maximum vocal intensity last as the patient is more likely to feel die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 alison thompson confident and relaxed at this stage. test by asking the patient to shout a word such as "help!" or "hullo" as loudly as possible. the rationale behind this test should be explained in order to elicit the patient's full cooperation. in spite of this, reliable results may not be obtained. after the assessment has been completed, the patient should be asked to evaluate his performance in relation to his average speech performance over the previous few days. it may be helpful to obtain a similar evaluation from the spouse or other family member in order to confirm the reliability of the patient's subjective impression. s p e e c h d i m e n s i o n s articulation parkinson's disease is characterized by indistinct articulation. the complex disorder of muscle functioning results in irregular contraction of muscles and an inability to initiate the co-ordinated contraction of several muscles at any given time. there is a failure to move the articulators through the full excursion necessary for clear phoneme production. alternatively, the patient may perform the articulatory movement adequately but fail to initiate phonation simultaneously. the indistinct articulation therefore reflects a failure to complete articulatory and phonatory movements correctly rather than the absence of movement. sarno1 8 found that the type or degree of phonemic dysfunction does not necessarily correlate with limitations noted in movements of the oral musculature. conversely, canter,5 investigated the relationship between diadochokinetic rate and articulation in parkinson's subjects and found that diadochokinetic rates for both tongue tip'and the back of the tongue correlated strongly with clarity of articulation. the correlation of lip diadochokinetic rate and articulation was slightly lower but still highly significant. standard single-word articulation tests are unsuitable for testing the parkinson's patient. research indicates that these patients usually perform normally on such tests despite the fact that they clearly have articulatory deficiencies in connected speech. 5 ' 9 canter5 evaluated the articulation in the context of connected speech and found that the majority of errors involved the plosives. these tended to be produced with a fricative quality and were at times omitted in final word position. logemann et al1 3 confirmed these findings and noted that errors of / v / is/ and 111 resulted from reduced constriction of the air channel. / the indistinct articulation within connected speech in many patients with advanced disease makes it impossible to determine precisely which phonemes have been elided, distorted or omitted. in severe cases speech becomes unintelligible to the point where word junctures cannot be determined by the listener. the latter is unlikely to be entirely due to inadequate articulatory contacts. factors such as low speech intensity, rapid speed of speech flow and akinesia contribute further to the listener's difficulty. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ratingscale of speech in parkinson's 45 ,· «f the iaw and associated facial muscles frequently result in poor rigidity oi tn j a r t i c u l a t e d s o u n d t h r o u g h the mouth. this lack of p r ° j e c f u r t h e r confounds articulatory intelligibility and has led to a n s s n of the speech as "swallowed" or "mumbled". rate of speech rpt · „ n o s s i b l y the most variable feature of parkinson s disease speech writers have stressed the occurrence of slowed rate of speech whust others have emphasized the abnormally rapid rate.2'* canter 3 t i n t e d out that many parkinson's patients have a speech rate which alls p,?fhin the normal range. of 134 parkinson's patients assessed clinically at the johannesburg general hospital, 44 (32,8 %) were found to have some «lowine of speech rate whilst 42 (31,4 %) had an increase in their rate of speech the remaining 48 (35,8%) patients fell within normal limits. clearly both slowed and increased rates occur as a result of a complex interaction of the many defects of muscle contraction produced by the disease each individual patient has a certain consistency in his overall rate of speech. as the disease progresses the fast speaker may become slower however, the reverse is uncommon. the term "propulsive rate" has been used to describe the abnormally fast rate of speech. it suggests the uncontrolled nature of the speech flow, similar to the propulsive gait, where the sequence of movement, once initiated, cannot readily be inhibited. in severe cases of slowed rate of speech the patient can be seen to expend a great deal of effort in talking. the speech appears laboured, with difficulty in moving from one articula'tory posture to the next. t h e patient frequently adapts to his difficulty by "editing" his speech, using only the minimum number of words necessary for communication. akinesia of speech akinesia can be defined as the inability to change from one muscle contraction pattern to another, to initiate or maintain an action in the face of the necessity to change. _ akinesia, as it relates to speech, presents as an inability to initiate articulation a n d / o r vocalization at the beginning of a speech segment or as a perseveration of an articulatory movement within a word or segment. the frustrated attempt to initiate speech may last several seconds. once blocking of the speech musculature has been overcome, articulation frequently occurs propulsively over the ensuing speeeh segment·. this segment may consist of several words, phrases or sentences. abnormally long intervals occur between segments, reflecting the patient's inability to initiate each consecutive segment. these intersegmental intervals frequently appear inappropriately and not at the natural point within or at the end of a sentence. t h e intervals consist of a freezing or posturing of the articulators. t h e posturing may be either totally silent or else be accompanied by vocalization. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 alison thompson if the patient initiates vocalization successfully he generally sustains this until the blocking of the articulators is released for production of the intended utterance. the perseverative repetitions would appear to occur most commonly on phonemes or phoneme clusters in the initial word position, but can also be noted in the medial word positions. perseveration of articulatory postures gives a stuttering type quality to the speech. where speed of speech is also very rapid there can be cluttering with random omission of phonemes, syllables or even words. akinesia should not be confused with dyskinesia which occurs as a complication of dopaminergic medication. dyskinesia presents as random choreaform movements of all muscle groups. perioral dyskinesia is common. there is forced opening of the mouth with associated writhing and twisting of the tongue, lip, and cheek muscles whilst the patient is at rest. movements may be inhibited to a certain extent during the actual speech act. vocal intensity a subjective impression of overall reduced intensity is generally accepted as one of the earliest signs of speech disturbance in the parkinson's disease patient despite laboratory research by canter 3 ' 4 indicating that there is no difference in average conversational intensity levels between parkinson's patients and normals. whilst he supports the fact that th'e speech of a parkinson's patient is frequently perceived clinically as having inadequate intensity, he suggests that there must be parameters of speech loudness other than average intensity level which lead to this perception. greene and watson8 have attributed the reduced vocal intensity to impaired respiratory movements which result in weak and fluctuating subglottic pressure. the parkinson's patient also demonstrates reduced voluntary vocal intensity at both ends of the intensity range. 2 ' 4 there is an inability to phonate very loudly or very softly. the former appears to be due to rigidity of respiratory and laryngeal musculature whilst the latter reflects the failure to maintain the necessary balance between subglottic pressure and vocal fold tension required for very low intensity phonation. reduced intensity range can be tested by asking the patient to both whisper and to shout a word as loudly as possible. it is a difficult feature to rate clinically owing to the considerable individual variation in maximum intensity amongst normals as well as the patient's self-consciousness in producing his best attempt. ^ for these reasons the writer has not found it practical to'attempt a fine clinical rating of intensity range, but rather to note whether the range appears to be within normal limits or is clearly outside the normal range. speech decay owing to the rigidity of the respiratory, laryngeal and oral musculature there is a tendency to weak or absent phonation and articulation at the end of speech segments. in advanced cases, where speech is slow and the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ratingscale of speech in parkinson's 47 laboured, speech decay may occur on the majority of words in the utterance. as the patient reduces effort, on approaching the end of a speech segment, the muscle rigidity prevents the fine control necessary to maintain phonation and firm articulatory contacts. described subjectively, the word endings appear to "trail off" and become inaudible. it would appear that maximum phonation times are reduced in the parkinson's patient although not all researchers are in a g r e e m e n t . 2 ' 4 ' 1 0 canter4 found that his patients were able to produce a sustained vowel for less than half as long as normal controls. speech decay, plus the poor phrasing of speech, may therefore be in part due to the inherent difficulty in sustaining phonation for an extended period of time. vocal quality rigidity and bradykinesia of the laryngeal musculature leads to incomplete adduction of the vocal folds. a hoarse, or harsh, breathy vocal quality is frequently the result. in some patients, this hoarse quality is constant and is superimposed on the entire speech flow whilst in others there is a random fluctuation between normal and hoarse quality. the latter case is due to continually alternating muscle tone and an inability to maintain the necessary fine motor co-ordination of the relevant muscle groups. previous research suggests that regular variability in intensity and/or frequency between adjacent vibratory cycles correlates well with perceptual judgements of vocal roughness or hoarseness. 1 2 ' 2 0 constant hoarseness should be distinguished from a fluctuating vocal quality which comprises episodes of hoarse speech interspersed with episodes of normal or near normal quality speech. t h e patient may present with either possibility at any given stage of his illness. as the disease progresses, the patient's vocal quality may change from fluctuating to constant hoarseness or vice versa. in advanced cases, the tendency is towards a constant forced hoarseness accompanied by marked speech decay. t h e parkinson's patient is, in practice, rarely seen to be totally aphonic. in some patients the development of aphonia may be delayed or prevented by suitable medical treatment. t h e patient may resort to whispering as an avoidance of effort, but this does not reflect his true phonatory potential. prosody dysprosody, or monotonous speech, relates to a lack of control over the stress and inflection variations natural to normal speech. it has been suggested that emphasis on stressed syllables and key words is reduced in parkinson's disease.7 dysprosody, together with reduced vocal intensity are frequently the earliest speech signs of parkinson's disease to be observed clinically. research indicates a lack of agreement as to the precise origin of the subjective impression of monotony. canter3 reports that reduced intensity die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 alison thompson variability (cf. stress) was not a feature of his parkinson's patients as compared to normals. he concluded that reduced use of stress is not responsible for the clinical impression of monotony and postulates that the reduced pitch range (cf. inflection) is at least partly responsible. dysprosody may not be entirely due to dysfunction of the peripheral speech mechanism. peacher1 6 thought that it reflected both central and peripheral disorders, whilst monrad-krohn14 considered it to be a central language disorder which impairs the melodic variations of speech. darley et al7 suggested that restricted range of movement was the most likely cause of the prosodic insufficiency. on clinical assessment, the patient's immobile face may well add psychological weight to the subjective impression of monotonous speech. the parkinson's patient may demonstrate a significantly higher modal pitch than normals, i.e. there is an increase in the average pitch level over the entire speech flow. subjectively, the patient appears to be speaking at a somewhat higher pitch than normal, quite apart from prosodic features which may or may not be intact. sarno1 8 and morley15 noted this trend as a result of perceptual evaluations. canter3 confirmed this clinical finding using frequency analysis. his patient group had a median fundamental vocal frequency of 129 hz on the average, whilst the median for the normal controls was 106 hz. clinically, an increased modal pitch is a difficult feature to assess. it can be identified far more readily in the male patient than in the female. this is due to the fact that there is a far greater permissible range for high modal pitch levels in the female than in the male. the phenomenon is probably attributable to a fairly constant rigidity of the entire laryngeal musculature. parkinson's disease speech rating scale emotional beka viour congruent confused lethargic depressed euphoric labile anxious unco-operative pitch other hearing saliva in mouth excessive normal reduced / effect on speech. pa tient s ra ting of: (a) fatigue level marked moderateminimal (b) physical status above average average below average the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ratingscale of speech in parkinson's 49 current medication time of a ssessment time of last dos a ge notes a . articulation 0 — no impairment. 1 — mild impairment of articulation. occasional indistinct plosives. 2 — moderate impairment. some distortion of fricatives. plosives frequently indistinct. 3 — severe distortion of fricatives with multiple indistinct or omitted plosives. difficulty in detecting word junctures. b. rate of speech 0 — no impairment. (i) fast speech: 1 — slight increase in rate. 2 — speech flow moderately fast. 3 — speech flow rapid. or (ii) slow speech: 1 — slight decrease in rate. 2 — moderate slowing of speech flow. 3 — marked slowing of speech flow. speech laboured. c . akinesia of speech (i) initiation of segments: 0 — no impairment. 1 — occasional difficulty in initiating articulation a n d / o r vocalization. slight lengthening of intersegmental intervals. 2 — frequent difficulty in initiating articulation a n d / o r vocalization. silent posturing of articulators observable plus longer intersegmental intervals. 3 — severe difficulty in initiating vocalization and/or articulation. silent posturing results in lengthy intervals between speech segments. (ii) perseveration: 0 — no impairment. 1 — occasional single repetition of phonemes a n d / o r phoneme clusters. 2 — frequent repetition of phonemes and phoneme clusters within a word. perseverations consist of one or two repetitions. 3 — severe perseveration of phonemes and phoneme clusters. repetitions are varied and lengthy. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 alison thompson d . intensity 0 — no impairment. 1 — overall speech intensity slightly reduced. 2 — overall intensity moderately below the normal range. weak vocalization still audible. 3 — speech aphonic. e. speech decay 0 — no impairment. 11 mild decay of vocal intensity and articulatory contact at the end of speech segments. 2 — moderate reduction in vocal intensity and articulatory contact at the end of segments. 3 — speech decay severe. word endings frequently unintelligible owing to poor articulatory contact. weak/absent vocalization. f . vocal quality 0 — no impairment, (i) constant: 1 — slightly hoarse quality superimposed on entire speech flow. 2 — moderate hoarse quality. 3 — forced, hoarse quality. visible effort expended in vocalizing. or (ii) fluctuating: 1 — occasional fluctuation in vocal fold quality resulting in moments of hoarse quality interspersed in a speech flow of normal vocal quality. 2 — approximately 50 % of the speech flow is produced with normal quality. hoarse quality interspersed randomly over words, phrases or sentences. 3 — hoarse quality predominates, but moments of normal vocal quality are still detectable in the speech flow. / g . prosody 0 — no impairment. 1 — slight dysprosody. speech slightly monotonous. 2 — speech monotony pronounced, but some use of stress and inflection still evident. j 3 — severe dysprosody. speech produced in a flat, unvarying monotone. i the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, j 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ratingscale of speech in parkinson's 51 h. pitch 0 — no impairment. 1 — modal vocal pitch slightly above the normal range. 2 — moderately raised modal pitch. 3 — marked rise in modal pitch. maximum vocal intensity within normal limits reduced unreliable other ra ting of speech a ssessment sa mple by patient: below average average above average by family member: below average average above average ackno wledgements the author wishes to extend grateful appreciation to prof. h.e. reef, chief neurologist of the johannesburg general hospital, for his assistance and guidance with this project. thanks to dr. j. mcmurdo, medical superintendent of the johannesburg hospital, for his permission to publish. references 1. blonsky, e.r., logemann, j., boshes, b„ and fisher, h. (1975): comparison of speech and swallowing function in patients with tremor disorders and in normal geriatric patients: a cinefluorographic study. j. geront., 30, 299-303. 2. boshes, b. (1966): voice changes in parkonsonism. j. neurosurg. suppl., 24, 286-288. 3. canter, g.j. (1963): speech characteristics of patients with parkinson's disease: i. intensity, pitch and d u r a t i o n . / speechhear. dis., 28, 221-229. 4. canter, g.j. (1965 a): speech characteristics of patients with parkinson's disease: ii. physiological support for speech. j. speech hear. dis., 30, 44-49. 5. canter, g.j. (1965 b): speech characteristics of patients with parkinson's disease: iii. articulation, diadochokinesis, and overall speech adequacy: j. speech hear dis., 30, 217-224. 6. darley, f.l., aronson, a.e. and brown, j.r. (1969 a): differential diagnostic patterns of dysarthria. j. speech hear. res., 12, 246-269. 7. darley f.l., aronson, a.e., and brown, j.r. (1969 b): clusters of deviant speech dimensions in the dysarthrias. j. speech hear. res., 12, 462-496. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 alison thompson 8. greene, m.c.l. and watson, b.w. (1968): the value of speech amplification in parkinson's disease patients. folia phoniat., 20 250257. 9. hoberman, s.g. (1958): speech techniques in aphasia and parkinsonism. /. mich. st. med. soc., 57, 1720-1723. 10. kreul, e.j. (1972): neuromuscular control examination (nmc) for parkinsonism: vowel prolongations and diadochokinetic and reading rates. j. speech hear. res., 15, 72-83. 11. leanderson, r., meyerson, b.a. and persson, a. (1971): effect of ldopa on speech in parkinsonism: an emg study of labial articulatory function. /. neurol. neurosurg. psychiat., 34, 679-681. 12. lieberman, p. (1963): some acoustic measures of the fundamental periodicity of normal and pathologic larynxes. /. of acoust soc amer., 35, 344-353. 13. logemann, j., blonsky, e.r., boshes, b„ and fisher, h. (1972): the steps in the degeneration of speech and voice control in parkinson's disease. in parkinsons disease, siegfried, j. (ed.), 2 hans huber, switzerland. 14. monrad-krohn, g.h. (1957): the third element of speech: prosody in the neuro-psychiatric clinic. j. mental science, 103, 326-331. 15. morley, d. (1955): the rehabilitation of adults with dysarthric speech. j. speech hear. dis., 20, 58-64. 16. peacher, w.g. (1950): the etiology and differential diagnosis of dysarthria. /. speech hear. dis., 15, 252-265. 17. rigrodsky, s. and morrison, e. (1970): speech changes in parkinsonism during l-dopa therapy:. preliminary findings. /. amer geriat. soc., 18, 142-151. 18. sarno, m.t. (1968): speech impairment in parkinson's disease arch. phys. med. rehab. 49, 269-275. 19. webster, d.d. (1968): critical analysis of the disability in parkinson's disease. modern treatment, 5, 257-282. 20. wendahl, r. (1966): laryngeal analog synthesis of jitter and shimmer auditory parameters of harshness. folia phoniat, 18, 98the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) r e p u b l i c i ' i t t e l e p h o n e 2 3 6 6 8 5 " p.o. b o x 5 2 0 4 1 s a x o n w o l d 2 1 3 2 r e p u b l i c | h e a r i n g a i d c o n s u l t a n t s ( p t y ) l t d , 7 3 2 m e d i c a l c i t y e l o f f c o r . j e p p e s t r e e t i | | | | 1 i 1 1 i i i i i i j o h a n n e s b u r g , t r a n s v a a l hearing aids. we specialize in the supply and fitting of hearing aids for all hearing losses, especially for nerve deafness recruitment bone conduction cases. cross aids: cros bicros multicros etc. bone conduction aids for body, earlevel, glasses (speciality by viennatone) b i n a u r a l fittings we import and stock : viennatone, qualitone, microson, phonak hearing aids. moulds: soft, hard, skeleton, vented, occluded etc. repairs: all aids supplied with a scientific performance report after repair. special prices for dealers and institutions. accessories: teacher pupil, parent child, individual audiotrainers, very reasonably priced. group audio trainers. tv wireless infrared transistor receiver sets made by sennheiser. audiometers: screening, diagnostic, research, era and electrocochleography c.o.r. and peep show. impedance bridges. manufacturers of sound proof booth and sound proof rooms. hearing aid testing set by " f o n i x " u.s.a. phonak noise generator with different frequencies, pure tone and warble tone, for everyday's use. we repair and calibrate audiometers. sound level meters, calibrators. industrial noise consultants. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 23 the use of bliss symbols as a first step into literacy with four children with down syndrome erna alant centre for augmentative and alternative communication department of communication pathology, university of pretoria,pretoria abstract this study describes the use of bliss symbolics as a first step into literacy with four children with down syndrome in a preschool setting. initial stages of the intervention programme are discussed as well as the children's ability to read bliss symbols six months after commencement of the programme. symbol errors are analyzed and implications for further research discussed. opsomming hierdie studie beskryfdie gebruik van bliss-simbole as eerste stap tot geletterdheid by vier kinders met downsindroom in 'n voorskoolse opset.inisiele stadiums van die intervensieprogram is bespreek asook die kinders se vermoe om die bliss-simbole te lees ses maande na aanvang van die program. simboolfoute is geanaliseer en implikasies vir verdere navorsing bespreek. literacy is an important skill which facilitates the integration of disabled people into society. this is not only because of the print-dominant society we live in, but also because exposure to print enriches the individual's language and facilitates further conceptual and metalinguistic abilities (blackstone, 1989; koppenhaver & yoder 1992; van kleeck| 1992). traditionally children who had no or limited verbal expression were involved in speech/language programmes and not in reading programmes as it was believed that they did not have sufficient language skills to read (koppenhaver & yoder, 1992). a consequence of this has been limited exposure of cognitively handicapped children to reading instruction, particularly in cases where children have a specific language learning deficit, for example down syndrome (stoel-gammon, 1990). recently, however, researchers have opposed this view, for example, raver & dwyer (1986) who demonstrated a relationship between the reading and language ability of the 5 preschool mentally handicapped children involved in a conversational approach to teaching. they argued that although it was typically believed that reading instruction must be postponed until a child's expressive language is "ready", the results of their study did not support this assumption. apart from limited exposure to instruction, the success of teaching literacy to cognitively handicapped people has been limited. the reasons for this failure are multiple, including, poor teaching strategies (kuntz, carrier & hollis, 1978; raver & dwyer, 1986), children's limited language ability (hern, smith & fuller, 1992; stoel-gammon 1990), limited level of abstraction and poor concentration (jeffree, 1981; matson & mulick, 1991). wishart (1990:250) maintained that many professionals still hold outdated conceptions of the nature of down syndrome and of its developmental implications and indicated that "sufficient evidence has already accumulated to suggest that with the appropriate support and input, children with down syndrome in future generations will undoubtedly fare better than previously". the nature of the support needed, however, constitutes a major point of controversy which has led to a critical review of present methods used to enhance literacy development. the methodology of teaching literacy to cognitively handicapped individuals has focused mainly on the teaching of sight vocabulary in order to give entrance into as broad a vocabulary of written words as possible. the underlying assumption is that auditory and visual processing of the cognitively handicapped person are delayed and that they would not be able to cope with the complex skills of analyses and syntheses in reading (for example word attack skills). cognitively handicapped people will therefore learn to read in the same way as cognitively-able people although at a slower rate with little focus on word attack skills. the teaching of sight vocabulary limits the person's reading to a number of words which is, however, no longer sufficient. folk & campbell (1978) pointed out that trainers must find techniques that will allow trainable cognitively handicapped children to read recipes, newspaper items, movie schedules, vocation information, store signs and so forth. "the question is not whether trainable level students die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 erna alant are competent enough to acquire basic reading skills, but whether we as special educators are competent enough to teach them." (folk & campbell, 1978:322). alternative teaching strategies are particularly important in view of the heterogeneity in language performance found in cognitively restricted children where a single systematic pattern of language and cognitive development is highly unlikely ( miller & chapman, 1984). kangas & lloyd (1988:211) support this argument by stating " ..a model of child development may not be a sufficient framework for selecting goals of intervention for children and adults who experience severely disabling conditions and who are functioning at early levels with respect to developmental norms". wishart (1990) agrees with this notion and stresses that particularly in the case of down syndrome, early learning styles differ fundamentally from those seen in non-handicapped children. the pathways and processes used to process information by the child are thus at the basis of the different methods and systems in teaching language and literacy skills. mcnaughton (1992) discusses that processing differences occur between children who are ablebodied and those who have speech and physical impairments and hypothesizes that the different pathways make a difference to the way in which they process information and thus influence their learning to read. children who use a visual system instead of speech for communication may be able to compensate for the limitations they face in speech production domains. this suggests that severely communicatively handicapped children can get access to reading through a different symbolic pathway than that of normal children as this may provide an easier transition to normal orthography (romski, sevcik, pate & rumbaugh, 1985). various authors have suggested that "bridging" between pictures and print seem to be an effective teaching strategy with cognitively handicapped individuals (jeffree, 1981; mcnaughton, 1992). apart from the children learning to read words quicker, more reading fluency is achieved with resulting positive confidence with the task (jeffree, 1981). different symbol systems have been used to enhance literacy skills in cognitively handicapped children by providing a "bridge" to normal orthography, e.g., rebus, makaton, bliss. the use of bliss symbols for entrance into literacy have been propagated particularly in view of the extensive vocabulary and range of symbols available that can be combined to create new concepts (burroughs, albritton, eaton & montague, 1990; shepherd & haaf, 1992). although bliss has been used extensively with cognitively handicapped children, there is little doubt that these symbols are more difficult to learn than some of the other symbol systems e.g., pics and rebus (mizuko, 1987). this feature which refers to the more abstract nature of some of the symbols could be seen as part of the reason why this system is an appropriate bridge into literacy. the combination of symbols also creates the opportunity for analysis and synthesis skills to develop on a visual level before these skills are required in normal orthography. there have, however, not been any studies done on the association between analysis and synthesis skills on a conceptual level (for example, bliss) and reading ability. at most this relationship could be defined as loose, although the basic concept of analysis and synthesis in relation to reading is introduced before facilitating these skills in normal orthography. one could argue that the teaching of a conceptually based symbol system before teaching normal orthography could delay the actual reading process. superficially, this may seem to be the case, as the child will take longer before "reading" normal orthography. the extended world knowledge and exposure to concepts will, however, contribute to easier and more extensive access to reading once exposed to the process. similarly, it could be said that exposure to bliss symbols and normal orthography simultaneously could lead to cognitive overload as children may have problems in retaining two symbol systems (light & lindsay, 1991). as one system is conceptually based, while the other is letter based, however, they refer to two different functional systems which may facilitate information processing (underwood, 1978). the process of reading instruction with symbols can be described in three phases: phase 1, the teaching of symbols; phase 2, the sequencing of symbols; and phase 3, the fading of symbols to expose normal orthography only (kuntz et al., 1978). this study aims to describe the initial phase of teaching new symbols to four children with down syndrome, followed by a description of the sentence reading they were doing 6 months later. a qualitative description of symbol errors is made and discussed in order to shed some light on the symbol acquisition of the children. method the goals of this study were firstly to describe four cognitively handicapped children's ability to learn bliss symbols within a ten week period in order to use bliss symbolics as an entrance into literacy. the subgoals were threefold: firstly to study the process of learning bliss symbols in terms of recognition and labelling, secondly to investigate the children's conceptual ability in relation to symbols taught during this period and thirdly, to describe the children's bliss reading six months after the final training period. finally a qualitative description of the symbol errors is made. \ subjects: | j four children with down syndrome enrolled at a nursery school for disabled children in pretoria were included in the study. all the children were in the same class in the preschool specifically geared to the needs of children with down syndrome (see table 1 for a description of the subjects). table 2 gives more detail on the present communication abilities of the children. procedure: the bliss symbols were taught as part of the regular preschool curriculum. the teacher selected the symbols according to the theme for the particular period. the number of symbols used each period varied according to the need identified by the teacher (see table 3). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the use of bliss symbols as a first step into literacy teaching of symbols: each theme was introduced in the same manner and the same school routine was used during the eight weeks of implementation. in the teaching of the symbols, particular steps were followed by the teacher which will be discussed below.all of these steps included teaching within a group setting.the daily routine during the eight weeks was as follows: with four children with down syndrome 25 * day 1: experiencing the concepts. all the children of the school went on an outing which was related to the theme of that week [see table 3 for an outline of the themes], the aim of the outings was to provide the children with the real experience and to bring them into contact with the actual objects.the teacher focused the table 1. description of subjects (n=4) characteristics subject 1 subject 2 subject 3 subject 4 chronological age [years & months] 7,2 5,7 5,5 3,7 mental age [years & months] 4,0 2,11 3,8 2,6 home language afrikaans afrikaans afrikaans afrikaans syndrome down down down down hearing normal normal normal normal previous exposure to bliss [months] 18 3 12 3 socio-economic status middle middle middle middle gender male male male female table 2. communication and visual perceptual abilities of the children evaluation measurement tools subject 1 subject 2 subject 3 subject 4 chronological age (ca) [ca:86 months] [ca:67 months [ca:65 months] [ca:43 months] receptive vocabulary *ppvt extremely poor extremely poor extremely poor receptive language *tacl sub tests: word'classes and relations grammatical morphemes elaborated sentences 1 *rdls comprehension 49-53 months 42-45 months 56-58 months 33 months 30-31 months 29-31 months 28-31 months 31 months expressive language *mlu j •predicted chronological age j >6 >58.3 months 2.00 26.9 months 5.6 55.2 months 1.9 26.1 months pragmatic behaviour •checklist of pragmatic behaviour. good average average poor cognitive development *dasi 2 developmental age 48 months 35 months 44 months 30 months visual perception •developmental test of visual perception •developmental test of visual motor integration 57.2 months 35.6 months 52.7 months not available ppvt: peabody picture vocabulary test; afrikaans translation by gouws (1975). tacl: test of auditory comprehension of language; afrikaans translation by kritzinger (1985). ca: chronological age. rdls: reynell developmental language scales (reynell & huntley, 1985). mlu: mean length of utterance; computation according to miller, 1981. pragmatic behaviour: good: 60-100% of communicative intents and conversational devices were observed; average: 30-60% poor: 0-30% dasi 2: developmental activities screening inventory (fewell & langley, 1984). developmental test of visual perception (frostig, 1963). developmental test of visual motor integration (beery, 1967). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 erna alant children's attention on concepts which were to be included in training and provided language stimulation throughout the outing. a video recording was made by one of the teachers, which was used in teaching later on in the week. no formal work was done on day 1, as the children left for home after the outing. no bliss symbols were thus introduced on day 1. * day 2:introduction of pictures, objects and symbols. on day 2 the class was introduced to the interest table. this table contained objects and/or toys of the vocabulary to be taught.the teacher and the children had an informal discussion about the objects and related it back to their outing. new concepts were explained, demonstrated and acted out where necessary, in order to highlight the meanings of the concepts. this table was displayed in the classroom throughout the day.after this the interest board was introduced containing the symbols and pictures.pictures were matched to the objects on the object table and then put onto the interest board, followed by the bliss symbol for that particular object/picture. the blissymbol-interest board was displayed in the classroom throughout the week.this procedure was followed with all the symbols to be taught. the bliss symbols were presented as follows: * global presentation of the bliss symbol on a flashcard * naming of the symbol * association of the symbol with the objects and pictures * identification of indicators [plural and action indicators] * as soon as all the new symbols were introduced, the teacher made short sentences with the bliss symbols and the children had to read them. new and old symbols were used in the sentences.the teacher also asked comprehension questions, where children had to match the correct picture to the sentence. drill practise: after the groupwork, the teacher worked with the children individually for approximately 10 minutes a day in order to reinforce the meaning of the symbols.the work done in school was sent home and it was expected of the parents to reinforce the sentences at home. the same procedure was followed for the remainder of the time period in which the theme was used.new symbols were introduced every day and objects/toys on the object board were added on a daily basis. testing of symbols for the study: * each child was tested individually on the different occasions at school. the symbols were taught over an extended period of 10 weeks, but due to school and public holidays the period of exposure varied for each theme. during this time some children were also absent for a number of days due to illnesses or personal reasons. see table 3 for detail about exposure time. * baseline testing (before intervention) : in the beginning of a new training period, each child was evaluated to determine if he/she was familiar with the concepts that were to be taught and whether he/she recognized or was able to label the bliss symbols that were going to be taught. three pictures or symbols were presented and the child was requested to point to the specific picture or symbol. as the speech of some of the children was unintelligible, the evaluation of the ability to label bliss symbols had to be adapted. the symbol was presented and the child was asked to label it or to match it with the picture. a choice of three pictures was given once again. items that were indicated by trial and error were repeated in order to increase reliability. at the end of each period of teaching, the same procedure was followed to evaluate the number of concepts learned as well as the children's ability to recognize and label newly taught symbols (middle evaluation). a final evaluation (two weeks after intervention stopped) of all the concepts and symbols taught since the begintable 3. symbols taught during each period period 1 period 2 period 3 period 4 theme: wild animals insects birds good habits period of exposure (days): 6 days 6 days 8 days 8 days bliss symbols: nouns elephant monkey lion giraffe rhinoceros hippopotamus bee butterfly grasshopper museum duck owl ostrich parrot dentist teeth hair vegetables fruit milk verbs swing sit run bath is fly jump swim y brush eat drink other big small on high fast i my the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the use of bliss symbols as a first step into literacywith four children with down syndrome 27 ning of the period was conducted to determine how much was retained over this period. finally, an analysis was made 6 months later to determine progress in terms of bliss reading. results and discussion figures 1, 2, and 3 represent the global results of all 4 c a n d i d a t e s over the eight weeks as regards their scores on the baseline, middle and final evaluations of the concepts introduced, ability to recognize and label bliss symbols. from this diagram it is clear that all four children showed an increase in their understanding of the concepts over the 8 weeks. it is interesting that two of the c a n d i d a t e s , subject 2 and 3, showed slightly less understanding (9% and 8%) in the final evaluation than in the middle evaluation. this decrease could be because concepts were not internalized well enough before proceeding to the new concepts resulting in the elimination from short term memory. from figure two it is evident that 2 candidates did not recognize any symbols before training started, while two candidates recognized 32% and 40% respectively. once again, it is clear that all the candidates scored better in the final evaluation. as in the previous figure, candidates 2 and 3 again showed a slight decrease (2% and 7 %) in performance from the middle to final evaluation. although a smaller decrease than in the conceptual evaluation, this could be explained against the background of information processing models, whereby recognition is seen as a much easier skill in the information processing process (light & lindsay, 1991). a similar pattern is observed in figure 3 where all the subjects showed an increase in the labelling of bliss symbols over the total period. although a labelling response was not only scored on a verbal level (the child could also label by matching the picture to the symbol), it was interesting to note that there was an increase of verbal labelling from one of the candidates. once again, however, subjects 2 and 3 as well as subject 1 in this case, showed a decrease in scores from the middle to the final evaluations. labelling, (being the most difficult of the three skills in terms of information processfigure 2. overall recognition of bliss symbols in baseline, medial and final evaluations. % 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 |= t/ i / 'a / / ̂ / / / i= |= a * / l / / 1 0 i 1 1 2 i 3 4 baseline medial final 40 97 100 0 95 93 32 100 93 0 84 96 subject baseline 7ζά medial i i 1 1 i 1 1 ii 1 final figure 1. overall conceptual performance of the subjects. subject • baseline έζά medial w final figure 3: overall labelling of bliss symbols in baseline, medial and final evaluations % 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 1 0 0 baseline medial final 29 95 8 2 baseline 7ζά medial i i i | i 1 1 1 1 1 final 0 95 85 i i / 29 100 83 subject 0 78 83 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 erna alant ing and production) was most affected by the two week time lapse before final testing. although bliss symbols have been criticized for being more difficult for children to learn than other symbol systems e.g., pics and rebus ( goosens, 1983; hurlbut, iwata & green, 1982), most of the children in this study were able to learn the different symbols. the retention of concepts and symbolic skills over eight table 4. qualitative description of subjects' symbol errors more complex less complex less complex more complex is λ φ say a 0 jump a r v grasshopper a a a r > run λ walk a eat a 0 a drink sit l λ ~ί chair | ^ eat
    i'7.49 6.54-8.21 0.32 relative latencies (msec) ^peak i-iii 2.01 1.69-2.41 0.15 peak iii-v 1.88 1.48-2.41 0.18 peak i-v 3.90 3.45-4.37 0.22 absolute amplitudes (μν) peak i j 0.17 0.08-0.36 0.05 peak v |0.24, 0.08-0.50 0.08 relative amplitude (μν) peak v:i 1.50 0.55-3.85 0.67 n.b. for peak iv, combined group n=54 table 2 above reflects the diagnostic reference data for the various latency and amplitude measurements obtained from the combined group (n=60). 1 there is consensus among researchers, viz. schwartz & berry, (1985), rowe, (1978), and chiappa et al.(l979), that absolute amplitude measures are not normally distributed; are highly susceptible to myogenic activity and noise levels; are difficult to replicate, and are easily influenced by minor alterations in recording techniques. consequently, the measurement of absolute amplitudes do not enjoy the stability and reliability of their latency counterparts (schwartz & berry, 1985). in this study, the mean peak i amplitude value was ο. 17 μν and that of peak v was 0.24 μν. chiappa et al. (1979), presented a mean peak amplitude value of 0,28 μν and a mean peak v value of 0.47μν. stockard et al. (1978), published a mean value of 0,23 μ ν for peak i and 0,35 μν for peak v. it is not clear that there are no close approximations between and among reported measures. these reported variations in amplitude measures between and among normal hearers may be attributed to the present system of signal averaging and use of artifact rejection (fernandes, 1989). theoretically, a wanted evoked potential is extracted from ongoing eeg by signal averaging and the use of artifact rejection. that is, by increasing the signal-to-noise ratio. waveform and amplitude build up is, therefore, a product of time-locked averaging together with the rejection of other contaminating artifacts, e.g. myogenic and other cerebral activity. it has been found that consensus among researchers on how much of averaging and/or artifact rej ection is required before a response is judged as acceptable or not, is lacking. according to hyde (1985), the choice of the number of clicks presented for averaging is often "based on popular consensus rather than on quantitative rationale". due consideration has not been given to the influence of differences in "internal noise levels" among normal hearers when reference data are established. that is, some normal subjects may have higher internal noise levels, requiring longer periods of averaging with greater number of averages within a trial before eliciting an appropriate response than subjects who have lower internal noise levels (hyde, 1985). therefore a choice of either 1048,200 or 2048 clicks to elicit a suitable averaged response may not be appropriate for all normal hearers. furthermore, since the amplitude of a response is partly dependent on the number of averages that occur in a trial, it is reasonable to assume that response amplitudes will differ between and among individuals. this therefore may account for the variability in amplitude measurements that are reported in the literature. similarly, the use and control of artifact rejection to eliminate unwanted noise is not consistent in studies that have reported on normal amplitude values. it is therefore not surprising to find variations in the reported amplitude values between and among studies. the consistent and approriate application of signal averaging and management of artifact rejection needs to be given careful attention in future research. attention needs to be focused on decisions pertaining to the: i. actual number of averages required in a trail (i.e. 1048, 2000 or 2048 clicks) before a response is regarded as representative of a "true neurogenic" response. ii. use and control of artifact rejection so that the final response is truely representative of the baer without being contaminated by other artifacts. a reasonable course of action, is to set the artifact rejection limits so that little of the "well behaved" (low variance) activity is rejected, while all of the high variance (bursts of electromyogenic noise) activity is. this may be done by "tuning" the rejection level while observing the displayed activity, so that only about 5-10% of the good activity is rejected. perhaps, the manufacturers of evoked potential systems need to incor/» suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommimikasirafityikhi/is. vol. .17, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 4 cyril d govender porate additional desirable features that will allow for the display of the input eeg during averaging, rejection of trails in which large voltage artifacts occur and an assessment of amplitude variability within an averaging run. the above may assist in establishing appropriate reference data for amplitudes which may be used routinely in baer interpretation. thereafter, such reference data should be applied widely to assess how otoneurologic pathologies influence the measures, and to document the obtained patterns for ongoing comparisons. d) relative amplitude the peak v:i amplitude ratio table 2 reveals, that the mean amplitude ratio obtained in this investigation was 1.50. this is consistent with the findings of chiappa et al. (1979); rowe (1978), and that of starr & achor (1975), who have all reported that a value greater than 1.00 be considered as normal. in order to detect abnormality, musiek et al. (1984), state that amplitude ratio should be less than 1.00. stockard et al. (1978), however, state that a complete absence of peak v in the presence of peak i is an indication of relative amplitude abnormality. differing in this opinion, starr & achor (1975), state that a peak v:i amplitude ratio ofless than 0,5 at 55 dbsl is abnormal. later in 1978, stockard et al. suggested that the peak v absolute amplitude value which is reduced by more than 3 sd from the normal mean, together with a peak i amplitude that is larger than peak v, and an inter-trial variation ofless than 10% are all necessary for the peak v:i amplitude ratio to be defined as abnormal. chiappa et al. (1979), agree with starr & achor (1975),in the looftheir 104 normal subjects displayed apeak i amplitude which was larger than peak v. the findings of this investigation are in part agreement with starr & achor (19 75), and with chiappa et al. (1979), since 12 subjects (5 females and 7 males) displayed peak i amplitudes which were larger than peak v, although the overall mean was 1.50. the observed differences in amplitude ratios appear to be due to normal variations that occur within and among normal individuals. this contention is in keeping with stockard et al.'s (1977), statement that "alterations of baer morphology in the absence of quantifiable latency or absolute amplitude abnormality are not considered abnormal per se, because of the variability of baer waveforms within and among normal individuals." however, schwartz & berry (1985), are of the opinion that there is a dearth of well documented literature concerning the use of the v:i amplitude ratio in a large pathologic population. they suggest that considerable research is needed on the confounding effects of such variables as stimulus polarity, repetition rates, filter characteristics, electrode sites etc., prior to the general use of this measure in clinical practice. the investigator concurs with the above recommendation. due consideration should also be given to inter and intra individual variations when examining amplitude data. furthermore, and improvement in signal averaging and artifact control may aid in resolving the issue of obtaining variable amplitude measures in normal hearers. separate diagnostic reference data for males and females in response to the suggestion made by several researchers, viz. stockard et al. (19 78), (19 79); jerger & hall (1980), and jerger & johnson (1988), that diagnostic reference data be established separately for males and females, the raw data was further treated to reflect this separation. table 3, reflects the means, ranges and standard deviations for the various baer measurements as obtained from 30 females and 30 males. on inspection and comparison of the mean absolute latency values obtained for the two groups, it is evident that for all six peaks, females tended to show shorter latency values than males. this is also evident for the peak i-iii and peak i-v relative latency values. the absolute and relative amplitude measures show no such differences, implying that there are no observable differences between sexes for these measures in this investigation. however, further research focusing on the appropriate use of signal averaging and artifact rejection may produce realistic amplitude measures in normal hearers. once this has been achieved, it is suggested that the effect of sex difference on amplitude measurements be reassessed. the question of whether there is a statistically significant sex difference effect on the normal baer, needs to be researched further. ι in the interim, the fact that there are observed latency differen-j ces between the sexes as seen in table 3, is supportive of the suggestion that separate diagnostic reference data be esta-i blished for the two sexes. the establishment of such data, would prevent the clinician from applying inappropriate sex| related reference data to interpret the baer. | / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) diagnostic reference data for the monaural brain-stem auditory evoked response (baer) 6 5 table 3: summary statistics for absolute and relative latencies in milli-seconds and absolute and relative amplitude measurements i n micro-volts of the monaural evoked baer in females (n=30) and males (n=30) baer measures (p=peak) statistical measures baer measures (p=peak) males (n=30) baer measures (p=peak) females (n=30) males (n=30) absolute latencies x range sd x range sd ρ i 2.06 1.87-2.19 0.07 2.11 1.92-2.29 0.10 ρ ii 3.02 2.71-3.29 0.14 3.4 2.79-3.37 0.14 ρ iii 4.08 3.71-4.52 0.18 4.16 3.87-4.50 0.16 ρ iv 5.10 4.42-5.83 0.37 5.17 4.58-5.52 0.21 ρ v 5.98 5.54-6.58 0.24 6.03 5.50-6.42 0.22 ρ vi 7.44 6.75-7.96 0.30 7.55 6.54-8.21 0.33 relative latencies ρ i-iii 2.00 1.74-2.41 0.16 2.03 1.69-2.37 0.15 ρ iii-v 1.87 1.48-2.41 0.18 1.87 1.56-2.29 0.17 ρ i-v 3.84 3.49-4.37 0.22 3.91 3.45-4.33 0.23 absolute amplitude ρ i 0.17 0.08-0.36 0.05 0.16 0.09-0.29 0.05 ρ v 0.25 0.09-0.44 0.08 0.22 0.08-0.50 0.08 relative amplitude ρ v:1 1.50 0.74-2.50 0.51 1.50 0.55-3.85 0.81 n.b. for peak iv: female no. = 26 male no. = 28 table 3 represents the means, ranges and standard deviations for the various baer measurements as obtained for the females and males respectively. conclusion diagnostic reference data were established for both the combined group (n=60) and separately for females and males. similarities an differences between this study and of those reported in the literature were noted and discussed. the similarities in absolute latency measures were attributed to close approximations between testing protocols used, whilst variations were primarily related jto, among other variables, differences in reference intensity levels and the polarity of clicks. despite the difference between this study and of those reflected in table 1, the absolute latency of peak v remained resistant to variations in stimulus, recording and "normal subject" variables. therefore this measure,appears to be robust and maybe reliably used in otoneurological diagnosis and for estimating hearing sensitivity. the relative latency values generated are consistent with those reported in the literature (see table 1), and this is attributed to the fact that click presentation rates used are similar, i.e. 10-12 clicks per sec., in each of the studies. it is therefore suggested that clinicians may confidently use these measures to assess otoneurological pathologies that may upset the conduction of impulses in the auditory periphery (e.g. multiple sclerosis), provided that the click rate used is 10 to 12 per sec. differences in amplitude measures between this study and among other studies were noted. these variations were, among other factors, attributed to the manner in which signal averaging and artifact rejection have been manipulated in obtaining the average baer. further research in this respect has been suggested. however, the relative amplitude value of 1.50 obtained in this study is consistent with those reported in the literature. this implies that the ra measure is less variable in normals and therefore, may be used as a more sensitive measure of brain-stem auditory function than absolute amplitude measures. in view of the demonstrated differences in reference data between and among clinics and laboratories, the writer is of the opinion that clinicians should exercise caution in using reference data established elsewhere, especially if reported testing protocols differ in stimulation, recording and normal subject variables, e.g. sex. the observation that there were differences between the sexes is strongly supportive of the suggestion that separate reference data be established for the sexes (stockard et al. 1978; jerger & hall, 19.80). this would allow for the accurate clinical interpretation of the baer obtained in the two sex groups. therefore it is recommended that each clinic generates its own reference data commensurate with its needs. furthermore, noting that this study fell short of giving due consideration to age-related data across the continuum, interaural latency differences, use of different repetition rates, stimulus intensity reference levels and click polarity, future research considering the above, needs to be conducted to extend the present reference data base. the need for consensus to be reached among researchers and clinicians with respect to test protocols used in bear testing cannot be overemphasized. perhaps, an international conference involving the various disciplines that use this test pror ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 6 cyril d govender cedure should be held, in order to formulate a standard guideline or protocol for the use of interpretation of the baer. this would facilitate inter-clinic and/or laboratory comparisons, and perhaps aid in resolving some of the controversies that exist in baer testing and interpretation. in the interim, it is important the researchers and clinicians clearly define the parameters of their test protocols in establishing reference data. in addition, such data should be applied within populations having known otoneurological pathologies to assess the extent to which the reference data is able to differentiate normal from pathological ears. the latter is also applicable to the reference data generated in this study. references american electroencepalographic society. guidelines for clinical evoked potential studies. journal of clinical neurophysiology, 1, 3-53, 1984. american national standards institute·.-standard criteria for permissibility of ambient noise during audiometric testing. ansi 53:1977 new york: american national standards institute, 1977. beagley, h.a., and sheldrake, j.b. differences in brain-stem response latency with age and sex. british journal of audioloyy, 12, 69-77, 1978. chiappa, k.h., gladstone, k.j., and young, r.r. brain-stem auditory evoked responses. studies of waveform variations in 50 normal human subjects. archives of neurology, 36, 81-87, 1979. coats, a.c. and martin, j.l. human auditory nerve action potentials and brain-stem evoked responses. archives of otolaryngology, 103, 605-622, 1977. cornacchia, l., martini, α., and morra, b. air and bone conduction brain-stem responses in adults and infants. audiology, 22,430437, 1983. fernandes, c.m.c. ent department, medical school, university of natal, personal communication, 1989. gastone, g.c., and grigg, m.m. auditory evoked potentials. in e. niedermeyer and f. lopes da silva (eds.), elect roencephaloyraphy: basic principles, clinical applications and related fields. baltimore: urban and schwarzenberg. 1987. govender, c.d. an investigation into the effects of sex difference and contralateral masking on the monaural auditory evoked response (baer) obtained in a group of normal hearing indian undergraduate university students. unpublished masters dissertation, university of durban-westville, 1989. hall, j.w. auditory brain-stem audiometry. in j. jerger {ed.), hearing disorders in adults. san diego: college-hill press, inc, 1984. hecox, k., and jacobson, j.t. auditory evoked potentials. in j.l. northern (ed.), hearing disorders. boston: little-brown, 1984. hyde. m.l. instrumentation and signal processing. in j.t. jacobson (ed.), the auditory brain-stem response, san diego: college-hill press, inc, 1985. jerger, j., and hall, j. effects of age and sex on auditory brain-stem response. archives of otolarynyoloyy 106, 387-391, 1980. jerger, j., and johnson, k. interactions of age, gender and sensorineural hearing loss on abr latency. ear and hearing, 9. 190-197, 1988. jewett, d.l., and williston, j.s. auditory evoked responses for fields averaged from the scalp of humans. brain, 94, 681-696, 1971. lazarus, t. annets handedness questionnaire. modified and adapted for s.a. use. unpublished, available from author, department of psychology, university of durban-westville, 1989. moore, e.j. effects of stimulus parameters. in e.j. moore (ed.), bases of auditory brain-stem evoked responses. new york: grune and stratton, 1983. musiek, f.e., and gollegly, k.m. abr in eighth nerve and low brainstem lesions. in j.t. jacobson(ed.), the auditory brain-stem response. san diego: college-hill press, 1985. musiek, f.e., kibbe, k., rackliffe, l., and weider, d.j. the auditory brain-stem response i-v amplitude ratio in normal, cochlear and retrocochlear ears. ear and hearing, 5, 52-55, 1984. picton, t.w. abnormal brain-stem auditory evoked potentials: a tentative classification. in r.q. cracco and i. bodis-wollner (eds.), frontiers of clinical neuroscience. evoked potentials, vol. 3. new york: alan r. liss inc, 1986. rosenhamer, h., lindstrom, b., and lundborg, j. on the use of click evoked electric brain-stem responses in audiological diagnosis, i. the variability of the normal response. scandinavian audiology, 7, 197-206, 1978. rowe, m.j. normal variability of the brain-stem auditory evoked response in young and old subjects. electroencephalography and clinical neurophysiology, 44, 459-470, 1978. schwartz, d.m., and berry, g.a. normative aspects of the abr. in j.t. jacobson (ed.), the auditory brain-stem response. san diego: college-hill press, 1985. starr,α., and achor, j. auditory brain-stem responses in neurological disease. archives of neurology, 32, 761-768, 1975. stockard, j.j., and rossiter, v.s. clinical and pathologic correlates of brain-stem auditory response abnormalities. neurology, 27, 316-325, 1977. , stockard, j.j., stockard, j.e., and sharbrough, f.w. non-pathologicl factors influencing brain-stem auditory evoked potentials.] american journal of electroencephalographic technology, 18,: 171-209,1978. ' ; r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 7 appendix i bear test protocol technical and procedural considerations stimulus : transducer electrodes evoked response audiometer electrode sites polarity repetition rate filter pass band sweep time time frame no. of clicks per trial no. of trials contralateral masking level of test ear stimulus artifact rejection i recording of responses / / test environment patient state clicks ιοομ sec. duration electrodynamic tdh-39p earphones housed in free field audio-cups. self-adhesive silver-silver chloride. cadwell quantum 84 positive fz high forehead negative ipsilateral mastoid ground contralateral mastoid alternating 11,29 per sec 100hz 3000hz 1 division = 1 msec 10 msec post-stimulus 2048 minimum-two to ensure waveform repeatability. 60dbhl kept constant at 70 dbnhl switched on by built in alps printer anechoic chamber electromagnetically screened low noise levels ansi (1979). appeared to be relaxed or asleep lying in a supine position on a standard patient couch. n.b.: a control run prior to stimulation was done to allow for comparing and identifying true responses. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) < */ * ν u v -sex m i<§ r' jspvs. •s t a l k i n g to p r o f e s s i o n a l s the needier westdeneorganisation p.o. box 28975 sandringham 2131 telephone (011) 485-1302/3/4/5' wm r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 signed lexical items in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf sonya warren, b(log) (pretoria) durban school, psychological centre, durban santie meyer, m(log) (pretoria) h.e.c. tesner, ma (pretoria) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of pretoria abstract the signs for 15 lexical items were video recorded and analyzedfor 40 congenitally deaf subjects (hearing loss greater than 91 db in the best ear) from an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf a uniform and relatively arbitrary sign system was found to be in usage within the school. comparing these signs with the signs used in an english oral residential schoolfor the deaf, it became apparent that only certain signs for lexical items were similar. furthermore, a marked difference between the signs for lexical items used by the deaf in the afrikaans residential school and the "standard south african signs" were found. opsomming die gebare vir 15 leksikale items is op videoband geneem en ontleed vir 40 kongenitaal dowe leerlinge (gehoorverlies groter as 91 db in die beste oor) in 'n afrikaanse skool vir gehoorgestremdes. die leerlinge word deur middel van die orale metode opgelei. daar is bevind dat daar 'n eenvormige en relatief arbitrere gebaresisteem in die skool gebruik word. enkele van die gebare toon ooreenkomste met die wat in 'n soortgelyke skool waar engels die voertaal is, gebruik word. daar is ook enkele ooreenkomste tussen die gebare vir leksikale items van die dowes in die afrikaanse skool en die "standaard suid-afrikaanse gebare". sign languages as commonly used among the deaf, are highly structured and organized systems and thus allow for communication equal to spoken languages. a sign language1 consists of a lexicon, grammatical rules and semantic characteristics, which enables one to express ideas and satisfy communicative needs (bonvillian, orlansky and novack, 1978). signs serve as the lexicon of this visibly transmitted language. a single member of a lexicon is a lexical item (i.e. a sign) which in a spoken language would be a word. many different types of signs are in existence: local, provincial, standard, conservative and puristic (stokoe, 1976 as cited by caccamise, ayers, finch and mitchell, 1978). analogous to the structure of the phonological system of oral language, are four parameters: hand configuration, hand orientation, movement of the hand and location where these occur, which arise from the patterned movements of the hands (klima and bellugi, 1980). these parameters are combined simultaneously to form either iconic signs, which visually resemble the referent, or arbitrary signs, which bear little or no resemblance to the referent (orlansky and bonvillian, 1984). both iconic and arbitrary signs used by different signers are not only non-uniform throughout the world, but are not necessarily standardized within many countries (battison as cited by stokoe, 1980a). caccamise et al. (1978), state that the standardization can only occur through consistency of sign use, that is, through public and institutional acceptance of the same sign, for the same meaning, by different users. to obtain this 'consistency' of use is not easy and often proves impossible, owing to differing sociological, demographical 'and cultural factors present in a country (fisher, 1982). south africa is a country where the above-mentioned factors are especially apparent. a diverse cultural heritage, as well as demographic and politically induced separateness of its ethnic groups, emphasizes these differences (penn, lewis, greenstein, 1984). these ethnic groups have their own culture, each differing from the other. © sasha 1986 it can therefore be assumed that a sign system will develop among the deaf in each ethnic group. penn et al. (1984) hypothesize that those south african sign languages that exist, contain as rich a vocabulary and arbitrary a structure as any language. however, because sign languages differ just as much as spoken languages differ from one another (markowicz, 1977, as cited by musselwhite and st. louis, 1982) 'consistency' of sign use in this country would be virtually impossible. it has been proposed by lewis (1983) that because of south africa's discrete educational policies, the separate signing systems will reflect the social group of those who use them. a study in respect of the english deaf group in johannesburg was therefore carried out (greenstein, 1983), to determine whether or not uniform signs were used within an english oral residential school for the deaf, and whether or not there was a divergence from these signs from the proposed 'south african' signs of nieder-heitmann (1980). these signs2 in the book "talking to the deaf' are presently being promoted as being representative of the signs used by the majority of the deaf in this country (rousseau 1980). results indicate that uniform'signs were used in the english oral residential school, as hypothesized, and that 75% of these did indeed differ from nieder-heitmann's (1980) proposed sign system. by attempting to investigate the afrikaans deaf cultural group's use of sign, this study will.also aim at providing further information concerning the use of'uniform' south african signs. the purpose of this study is to determine whether or not certain lexical items in the sign lexicon used by the afrikaans-speaking deaf school child in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf, are uniform and consistently used within the school and to what degree this afrikaans sign lexicon differs from that of the english culture and the south african signs of nieder-heitmann (1980). in this way an indication of the degree to which a 1 such a language is american sign language (asl), a bona · fide language most widely used by the deaf in america. 2 these signs will be referred to as the south african signs. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lexical items in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf 57 consistently used uniform sign system exists within south africa, can be provided. method 1. aims the following aims were formulated: a . to describe the signs used by children in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf, determining whether or not the use of the signs described is uniform within the school. b. to determine whether or not the signs in the afrikaans residential school for the deaf, differ from those in use at the english oral residential school for the deaf, thus determining whether the nature of the signs is affected by the language culture group (greenstein, 1983). c. to determine whether or not the signs used within the school, differ from those proposed by nieder-heitmann (1980) to be the standard3 south african signs. 2. subjects for this study forty pupils aged 7 to 19 years were selected from an afrikaans residential school for the deaf. profound congenital hearing loss was present in all subjects (hearing level greater than 91 db in the best ear). the rationale for studying these subjects is based on evidence that they rely to a greater extent on non-verbal communication than subjects with more residual hearing (siple et al. 1978a). subjects with deaf parents were excluded from this study because these parents could influence the vocabulary used by the child. furthermore, subjects with other handicaps were also excluded because these handicaps could affect their signing abilities. the subjects selected for this study are presented in table 1. table 1: description of subjects variables age groups (years) 7-9 10-13 14-16 17-19 degree of loss: >91 db(hl) 10 / 10 10 10 onset of loss: congenital j10 10 10 10 hearing status of parents: normal 10 10 10 10 educational environment: afrikaans 10 10 10 10 residential status: boarders 10 10 10 10 secondary handicaps: none ! 0 0 0 0 intelligence: normal !10 10 10 10 sex: male ! 5 5 5 5 female ' 5 ' 5 5 5 3. material twelve of the fifteen lexical items were selected from those used by greenstein (1983) to compare the signs of the afrikaans and english deaf pupils. these are: nouns: hond, mamma, boom verbs: bad, spring, sit adjectives: geel, oud, bly prepositions: op, voor, na . . . toe in addition the following three emotive words were arbitrarily chosen: lag, kwaad, huil (warren, 1985) 3 standard = consistent use of signs (caccamise, ayers, finch and mitchell, 1978). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 4. representation o f test material a clear, colourful picture, representing each lexical item was used to elicit a response. below the picture was the printed form of the word in isolation as well as in a sentence in dark block lettering underneath the picture-word card (warren, 1985). 5. procedure a quiet, well-lit room in the school was used. instructions were given orally, in natural gestures and in sign, by the investigator. the subjects had to sign the word represented by the pictureword card. subjects were then individually tested to ensure that they could not influence one another's use of signs. subjects were filmed so that the whole body of the subject was video taped, capturing the total movement involved in producing the sign (warren, 1985). 6. scoring procedure for the analysis of data, the three parameters as described by stokoe (1980b) were used, namely: dez, sig & tab. in addition to these three classic parameters, orientation was analysed as a fourth parameter as suggested by battison, markowicz and woodward (1975, as cited by daniloff and vergara, 1984). orientation is important in sign formation, as it distinguishes between minimal pairs of signs. signs were analyzed according to: a. designation — dez — the distinctive handshape used to make the sign e.g. flat hand. a list of handshapes used by the subjects in this study were obtained from niederheitmann's book 'talking to the deaf (1980, p. 54) as a comparison of the signs of afrikaans subjects to the south african signs was being made, and a common classification system was needed. any handshapes used by the subjects, and not listed by nieder-heitmann (1980) were obtained from klima and bellugi (1980). b. signation — sig — the movement involved in making the sign, e.g. circular. c. tabulation — tab — the location where a sign begins and ends in relation to the signer's body. termed 'place of articulation' e.g. chest. d. orientation — orient — planes of the palms of the hand, e.g. palm up. results table 2 provides a clear description of the signs consistently used by the majority4 of afrikaans subjects — 50% being a significant majority according to penn and saling (1983 as cited by greenstein, 1983). fourteen of the fifteen signs have been described according to the four parameters dez, sig, tab (stokoe, 1980b) and orient (markowicz and battison, 1975 as cited by daniloff and vergara 1984). one sign, 'in front of is discussed in table 3, as no single sign is used by the majority for this lexical item. the percentage of subjects using the sign has been provided to indicate the extent to which each sign is used by the subjects. 4 signs used by the majority in the school will be referred to as uniform signs. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) , q sonya warren, santie meyer and h.e.c. tesner j o table 2: a description of the signs used by the majority of subjects in the afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf word % of ss dez sig tab orient dog (hond) 100% compressed hand opens and closes lower face vertical — unilateral opposite mommy* (mamma) 92,5% claw hand move hand across chest l-r, r-l chest vertical — toward body tree (boom) 95% cupped hands (bilateral) finger tips together move apart in a circular shape joining once more at wrists fingers wrist vertical — bilateral opposite yellow (geel) 85% broad u-hand wrist action opening of ear on same side vertical — unilateral opposite old (oud) 60% crooked fingers move finger slowly down cheek cheek on same side vertical — away from body happy* (bly) 57,5% flat hands (bilateral) clap hands together neutral vertical — bilateral opposite bath (bad) 95% spread hand rub hand in circular motion lower chest abdomen vertical — toward body jumping (spring) 72% v hand raise hand vertically from palm of opposite hand palm of opposite hand vertical — unilateral body sitting (sit) 77,5% fist hand move hand downward to strike palm of opposite hand palm of opposite hand vertical — unilateral opposite on (op) 72,5% flat hand move hand straight down neutral abdomen horizontal — palm down in front (voor) minority use — see table 3 to (na... toe) 50% first finger (bilateral) move 1st finger of dominant hand along side of opposite 1st finger side of opposite finger horizontal — p a l m down laugh (lag) 67,5 % clawed hand (bilateral) move hand l/r-r/l in front of lower face vertical — toward body cross (kwaad) 82,5% clawed hands (bilateral) no movement cheeks vertical — toward body cry* (huil) 85% v hand move fingers straight down cheeks cheeks vertical — toward body χ= 73% x = the average number of subjects using the sign system *signs considered to have a certain amount of iconicity as is evident from table 2, the sign for the lexical item 'dog' was the only sign consistently used by 100% of the subjects. ten of the fifteen signs were used by more than 70% of the subjects, while four of the signs, i.e. 'happy', 'to', 'laugh' and 'old' were used by 50% or more of the subjects. it was hypothesized that a sign system would be in use at the afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf. it is apparent from the results in table 2 that a certain uniform and largely arbitrary sign system is in existence within the school. it is considered uniform as most of the signs are consistently used by the majority of subjects. these signs which serve as a means of communication amongst the subjects, do not appear to be idiosyncratic to each individual signer, but have rather become a system commonly used by the majority of the subjects. this is in agreement with baker and cokely's (1980) proposal that the members of a community must agree on the meaning of symbols and the manner in which they are used for effective communication: it is apparent that the above-mentioned activities have taken place amongst the afrikaans subjects, owing to the fact that 73% (see table 2) of the subjects used the same signs for the representation of certain lexical items. / many of the signs in table 2 are also considered arbitrary as they do not visually represent the referent e.g. 'yellow'. however, baker and cokely (1980) state that degrees of arbitrariness exist where the sign, although largely arbitrary, has a certain degree of iconicity, i.e. relatively arbitrary. these signs are indicated in table 2 with the use of an asterisk e.g. 'mommy'. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) signed lexical items in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf table 3: a description of signs used by a minority of the subjects word % of ss dez sig tab orient happy (bly) 22,5% flat hands (bilateral) clap hands together palm of opposite hand horizontal — palm down jumping* (spring) 15% flat hands (bilateral) raise hands upwards. simulate jumping movement of legs side of body horizontal — palm down sitting* (sit) 15% flat hands (bilateral) move hands downwards. simulate sitting movement by bending knees side of body horizontal — palm down on (op) 17,5% flat hand (bilateral) clap hands together palm of opposite hand horizontal — palm down in front of (voor) 42,5% a. 35% b. cupped hand move hand forward in semicircle away from chest chest neutral vertical — toward body in front of (voor) 42,5% a. 35% b. first finger point finger forward — wrist of dominant hand bangs against opposite wrist wrists vertical — away from body to* (na..toe) 27,5% a. 20% b. fist hand move hand outwards in bold movement chest vertical — unilateral opposite to* (na..toe) 27,5% a. 20% b. fist hand point finger in front neutral region vertical — unilateral opposite laugh* 15% flat hands (bilateral) hand hold stomach — slight move up and down neutral vertical — toward body cry* (huil) 10% first finger (bilateral) move fingers straight down cheeks cheeks vertical — toward body χ = 6% percentage of subjects using the iconic* signs χ = 15% percentage of subjects using the minority system of signs •an asterisk marks the signs that are largely iconic. ι table 3 indicates the signs which are neither idiosyncratic to just one individual signer, nor representative of a majority use, but are, however, used by a minority, i.e. less than 50% of the subjects. the description of sign follows the same format as that used in table 2, while an asterisk marks those signs that appear to be iconic. pertinent to table 3 is the fact that all the signs were used by a minority of subjects — appearing to be less than 30% in all cases except for 'in front of. for the lexical item 'in front of, two differing signs, each used by a minority of subjects (42,5% and 35% respectively) were elicited (see table 3). the sign for 'in front of (a), subjectively viewed, appears motorically easier and slightly more iconic than 'in front of (b). since the lexical items that could be influenced by the context e.g. 'on', were placed in an appropriate sentence, it is felt that the own interpretation of the context by the subjects did not influence the form of the sign. (for example, "he sits on the chair"). it therefore appears that two signs are in use at the afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf, for the lexical item 'in front of. an issue of importance is the fact that 50% of the signs — five of the ten in table 3 — are iconic e.g. 'sitting' — (indicated by an asterisk) while the remaining five are relatively arbitrary. although table 3 indicates minority use, at least 10% of the subjects used the sign in each case. it is therefore apparent that these signs are not idiosyncratic to each individual, but rather serve as a communication system. thus it is evident that within this oral school for the deaf a uniform sign system used by a majority, as well as certain signs by the minority, (x 15%) are in existence. the uniform system shows consistent use by 73% of the subjects. this is in accordance with lunde (as cited by stokoe, 1980a) who states that although oral schools emphasize speech reading and speech, the fact is that the deaf, as a group, use sign language amongst themselves. 2. differences in structure of the signs used in the english and afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 sonya warren, santie meyer and h.e.c. tesner table 4: the variations across the four parameters which occur in the signs of the english and afrikaans subjects word dog mommy tree yellow old* happy* bathing jumping sitting on in front of to dez sig tab orient % afr ss 60% 57,5% 17,5% % eng ss 95% 77,5% 80% 92,5% 92,5% 92,5% 80% 70% 90% 50% extreme variations of english signs χ ι χ ι x 20% 87% the percentage of afrikaans subjects using the signs of the english subjects 13% * = signs are the same χ = variation in parameter table 4 is a comparison of the signs used by the majority of subjects in the afrikaans school and those used by the majority of subjects in the english school. the signs have been analyzed according to the parameters dez, sig, tab and orient. notable differences between the two sets of signs are indicated by a cross. the second to last column from the right indicates the percentage of afrikaans subjects using the signs that are used in the english oral residential school, while the last column in table 4 indicates the percentage of english subjects using each sign of the sign system, unique to the english oral residential school for the deaf. the sign for 'in front of could not be compared, as greenstein (1983) could also not determine a definite use of sign by a majority, for this lexical item. the' results in table 4 can be discussed after the division of signs has been explained. after researching the development of signs it was evident that there are differences between the younger and older subject's signs. greenstein (1983) noted that a development of ceftain signs (from the iconic to the more arbitrary forms) was evident. both the iconic and relatively arbitrary signs were treated as being part of a uniform system. in this study the signs were subdivided into the relatively iconic (developing signs, marked by an asterisk in table 3) and relatively arbitrary levels (see table 2). for comparison of the use of iconic and arbitrary signs by different age groups, the reader is referred to greenstein (1983) and warren (1985). although not proven, it is felt that the younger subjects will acquire the adult form of the sign (bornstein, 1978) (i.e. the more arbitrary uniform sign system). for this reason only the relatively arbitrary sets of signs are compared with greenstein's (1983) results. table 4 illustrates that only in two cases the same signs were used by the majority of english and afrikaans subjects; these were the signs for 'old' and 'happy'. the four signs 'dog', 'tree', 'sitting' and .'to' show a difference across all four parameters indicating an extreme variation for these lexical items in the two sets of signs. the remaining five signs 'mommy', 'yellow', 'bathing', 'jumping' and 'on' show at least one parametric variation. although the uniform signs of the afrikaans subjects for 'on' differed from the sign of the english subjects by three parameters, a minority of afrikaans subjects, i.e. 17,5% did use the sign used by 50% of the english subjects. similarly, 20% of the afrikaans subjects used the same sign for 'to' as was used by 87% of the english subjects. it must be noted that whereas the signs for 'on' and 'to' were used by a majority in the english school, they were only used by a minority in the afrikaans school. the sign for the lexical item 'jumping' used by the afrikaans subjects, was found to vary from the sign used by the english subjects, with 'palm up' as opposed to a 'palm down' orientation of the non-dominant hand (see appendix). although the three parameters dez, sig and orient correlated, the sign differed because of tabulation. the sign of the afrikaans subjects for 'bathing', differed only in 'movement' from the otherwise correlate english sign, while 'mommy' differed only through 'handshape' variation. it was hypothesized that the signs used by the majority of subjects in the afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf, would differ from the signs described to be of uniform use (of the majority) in the english oral residential school. the results therefore support the hypothesis that although both schools are using a uniform system of signs, the majority of these signs differ between the two schools. it has been said that children in a given school will invent and utilize signs not found elsewhere (cokely and gawlick, 1974 as cited by bornstein, 1978). bearing in mind that sign language has never been formally taught in white south african schools, it can therefore be expected that the pupils in both the english and afrikaans schools have devised their own system of signs. the fact that the english and afrikaans groups are representative of different cultures (baker and cokely, 1980) could explain these differences. furthermore, these differences could also be attributed to geographic areas. this is in accordance with markowicz (1980) who states that in different geographical areas different signs are sometimes used to represent the same thing. 3. differences in structure of the south african signs and the signs used in the afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf table 5: differences across the parameters occurring in two sets of signs word dez sig tab orient % afr ss dog x x x x mommy a) x x x b) x x x tree a) x x x x i b) x x x x j yellow x x x x 1 i •old 60% happy x 22,5% bathing x x x •jumping 72,5% sitting x x x x on x x x in front of a) x x x b) x x x x to x x x x percentage of afrikaans subjects using the south african signs / 13% * = signs are the same χ = variation in parameter a) and b) = two varying sa signs for the same lexical item. table 5 indicates the variations across the four parameters (i.e. dez, sig, tab and orient of signs) which occur between the south south african journal of communication disorders, vol 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) signed lexical items in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf a f r i c a n signs and the uniform signs used by the majority of subjects in the afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf. the analysis follows the same format as that of table 4. the far right hand column indicates the number of afrikaans subjects using the south african signs. for a detailed description of the differences in the two sets of signs refer to the appendix. in table 5 it is evident that, of the twelve signs used by the majority of afrikaans subjects, only two, namely 'jumping' •72,5%) and 'old' (60%) correlate exactly with the south african signs, for the representation of the same lexical items. the signs of the afrikaans subjects for 'tree' (b), 'dog', 'yellow', 'sitting', 'in front of (b) and 'to' show variations across all four parameters and therefore have no correlation with south african signs i.e. 'mommy' (a and b), 'tree' (a), 'bathing', 'on', 'in front of, while the sign for 'happy' is the only sign which correlates across three parameters with the south african signs. divergence is evident in the fourth parameter — orientation. the south african sign for 'happy' was, however, used by a minority (22.5%) of the afrikaans subjects, correlating across all four parameters because of orientation being 'palm down' as opposed to bilateral opposite which was used by the majority of afrikaans subjects. deaf culture — did not make consistent use of the signs proposed by nieder-heitmann (1980) to be representative of all signs used by various ethnic groups in south africa. the signs showed a marked difference from one another. this investigation therefore supports lewis' (1983) hypothesis that deaf populations in the country, who stem from discrete language and educational backgrounds, will exhibit divergence from the hypothesized standard sign system. this study is an important introductory contribution with regard to the investigation of the sign lexicon used by subjects in an afrikaans school for the deaf and adjunct to the study carried out by penn et al. (1984). with the exception of these results very little research is available regarding this specific sign system. it is an important research area as stokoe (1980b, ρ 126) states: 'sign languages generally and sign in particular make excellent objects for scientific study . . . for sign is a language which can make a deaf person a sharer in culture and also a member of a specific group with its own self awareness and pride.' this is specifically relevant in the demographically diverse multi-cultural situation in south africa.' the results of table 5 indicate that minority of subjects in the afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf are using the south african signs i.e. 13%. this clearly shows that within this school a sign system which differs from that of the proposed south african signs of nieder-heitmann (1980) is in existence. only one south african sign 'old' is used consistently by both english and afrikaans subjects and appears to be the only uniform south african sign of the twelve lexical items. rousseau (1980) describes the signs presented in niederheitmann's book as a systematized language system, incorporating the signs commonly used by the deaf in south africa. with these significant differences present it is evident that the validity of rousseau's (1980) proposal could be queried. furthermore, consistency is recognized as a critical factor, the basic premise upon which standardisation rests (caccamise et al. 1978). yet, it appears from the! above results that the south african signs are not consistently used by subjects to represent the lexical items tested. conclusions the findings support the hypothesis that a uniform and relatively arbitrary sign system is in existence within the afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf. the majority of subjects have a sign system which is not idiosyncratic, but is representative of the group studied as a whole. this is in accordance with literature where it is suggested that children in an oral school utilize a sign language amongst themselves (lunde as cited by stokoe, 1980b). although the white deaf population in this country are not taught sign language, they appear to 'turn quite naturally to their own language' (furth cited by markowicz, 1980). on comparison of the results of this study with an investigation carried out on english subjects in an english oral residential school for the deaf (greenstein, 1983) it was evident that single lexical items were similar. however, in most cases the signs of the afrikaans subjects were part of a sign system in existence within the afrikaans oral residential school, which is largely unique to the school. this is in accordance with cicoural (1978) who notes that a variety of sign forms emerge among signers of differing educational backgrounds. the results indicate that the subjects in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf — a subgroup of the south african references baker, c. and cokely, d. american sign language: a teacher's resource text on grammar and culture.!.j. publishers, inc., maryland, 1980. bonvillian, j.d., orlansky, m.d. and novack, l.l. developmental milestones, sign language acquisition and motor development. child development, vi. 54, 1435-1445, 1978. bornstein, h. sign language in the education of the deaf. in: i.m. schlesinger and l. namir (eds) sign language of the deaf. academic press, inc. new york, 1978. caccamise, f., ayers, r., finch, k., and mitchell, m. signs and manual communications systems: selection, standardization and development. american annals of the deaf vol 123, 887-901, 1978. cicoural, a.v. sociolinguistic aspects of the use of sign language. in: i.m. schlesinger and l. namir (eds.) sign language of the deaf. academic press, inc., new york, 1978. daniloff, j. and vergara, d. comparison between the motoric constraints for amer. ind. and a.s.l. sign formation. journal of speech and hearing research. no. 27, 70-76, 1984. fisher, s. sign language and manual communication. in: g.g. walter and r.l. whitehead. deafness and communication; assessment and training. williams and wilkins, baltimore, u.s.a., 1982. greenstein, a.l. signs used by pupils in an oral residential school for the deaf. unpublished thesis, university of the witwaterrand, johannesburg, 1983. klima, e. and bellugi, u. the signs of language. harvard university press, massachusetts, 1980. lewis, r.e. an overview of the research into the american sign language continuum and its relevance to south african sign languages and codes. an unpublished paper, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, 1983. markowicz, h. some sociolinguistic considerations of american sign language. in: w.c. stokoe. sign and culture. linstok press, maryland, 1980. musselwhite, c.r. and st. louis, k. communication programming for the severely handicapped: vocal and non-vocal strategies. college hill press, houston, texas, 1982. nieder-heitmann, n. talking to the deaf. government press, pretoria, 1980. orlansky, m.d. and bonvillian, j.d. the role of iconicity in early sign language acquisition. journal of speech and hearing disorders, vol. 49, 287-292, 1984. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 sonya warren, santie meyer and h.e.c. tesner penn c lewis, r. and greenstein, a. sign language in south africa: some research and clinical issues. south african journal of communication disorders, vol 31, 6-11, 1984. rousseau, g.j. foreword. in: n. nieder-heitmann. talking to the deaf government press, pretoria, 1980. siple, p., hatfield, n. and caccamise, f. the role of visual perceptual abilities in the acquisition and comprehension of sign language. american annals of the deaf. nov. 78, 852856, 1978a. siple, p. linguistic and psychological properties of american sign language: an overview. in: ρ siple (ed.) perspectives in neurolinguistics and psycholinguistics. a series of monographs and treatises. academic press, inc., new york, 1978b. stokoe, w.c. sign and culture. linstok press, maryland, 1980a. stokoe, w.c. the study and use of sign language. in: r.l. schiefelbusch. language intervention. university park press, baltimore, 1980b. warren, s. signed lexical items in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf. unpublished blog thesis, university of pretoria, pretoria, 1985. afrikaans subjects english subjects south african signs 100% 95% hond dez compressed hand (unilateral) 1st finger (unilateral) flat hand (unilateral) (dog) sig hand opens and closes finger moves from a central pat thigh with hand position to the centre of the throat tab centre of mouth centre of throat thigh orient vertical-unilateral vertical-toward body vertical-toward body face signer is required to look down while eliciting the sign 93% 77,5% mamma dez claw hand flat hand a b (mommy) m-hand flat hand sig move hand across chest move hand across chest tapping move across l-r or r-l l-r or r-l movement chest l/r and r / l tab chest chest — palm of the opposite palm of chest — point hand rests on chest opposite hand of contact is side of 4th finger orient vertical — toward body vertical — toward body vertical — horizontal — toward body palm up 95% 80% boom dez cupped hands (bilateral) spread/clawed hand a b (tree) spread hand spread handelbow bent sig finger tips together, move hand moves from side move hands move hand apart in a circular shape joining up and down from side to once more at wrists indicating the side ; outline of ; a tree 1 tab finger tips — wrists elbow rests in opposite shoulders — elbow rests on cupped hand abdomen back of opposite hand orient vertical — bilateral opposite vertical — toward body vertical — orientation bilateral changes as a opposite result of hand rotation face 87% 92,5% / geel dez u-hand y-hand 1st finger (yellow) sig wrist action of u-hand wrist action of y-hand tap 'opening' of ear on same side tab next to opening of ear on inside of opposite cup-hand 'opening' of ear on same side same side orient vertical-unilateral opposite horizontal-palm down vertical-toward body appendix: a description of the signs used by the majority of subjects in the afrikaans and english schools respectively together with a representation of the south african signs. south african journal of communication disorders, vol 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) signed lexical items in an afrikaans oral residential school for the deaf 63 afrikaans subjects english subjects south african signs 80% 92,5% oud* (old) dez sig tab orient face crooked finger move finger slowly down cheek cheek on same side vertical-palm away from body 57% 92,5% bly (happy) dez sig tab orient face flat hands (bilateral) clap hands together neutral region (abdomen) vertical-bilaterial opposite happy look flat hands (bilateral) clap hands together abdominal region vertical-appears bilateral opposite smile — happy look flat hands (bilateral) clap hands together abdominal region horizontal — palm down and up smile — happy look 95% 80% bad (bathing) dez sig tab orient face spread hand-unilateral rub hand in circular motion lower chest-abdomen vertical-toward body spread hand-unilateral rub hand up and down chest-abdomen vertical-toward body clawed fists (bilateral) rub hand up and down chest region vertical-toward body 72,5% 15% 35% 35% spring (jump) dez sig tab orient face a b v hand flat hands raised hand flat hands once, vertiare raised upcally from wards. jump palm of oppoup and down site hand (lower limbs) palm of opposite hand verticalhorizontaltoward body palms down a b v hand hands passive jumping jump up and movement down (limbs) hands at either side of body back of hands on opposite hand either side of body verticaltoward body v hand raise hand vertically from palm of opposite hand palm of opposite hand vertical-toward body 77,5% 15% 52,5% 22,5% sit (sitting) / / dez · 1 1 sig tab ! orient face a b fist hand flat hands move hand flat hands downward to move downstrike palm of ward. sitting opposite hand movement by bendingknees palm of opposite hand verticalhorizontal unilateral palms down opposite a b clenched fists, clenched fist bend elbows bend elbows bend elbows twist wrists back and bend back bend knees arms at side side of body of body verticalaway from body clenched fists — open thumb close fist by pushing down thumb side of body vertical — bilateral opposites 72% 50% op (up) dez sig tab orient face flat hand move hand straight down neutral— abdomen horizontal — palm down palm of flat hand clap palms together palm of opposite flat hand horizontal — palm down palm of flat hand clap palm of flat hand on back of opposite hand back of opposite flat hand horizontal — palm down "the only south african sign consistently used by english and afrikaans subjects. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 sonya warren, santie meyer and h.e.c. tesner afrikaa ns subjects english subjects south african signs 42% 35% variations voor dez a b in front cupped 1 finger extreme variation within compressed hand of) hand this sign compressed hand sig move hand point move hand across chest forward in finger forl-r or r-l semi-circle ward. wrist away from of dominant chest hand strikes wristof opposite hand tab chest neutral wrists chest orient verticalverticalverticaltoward body toward away from verticaltoward body body body face 50% 20% 87% na...toe dez a b (to) 1st finger 1st finger 1st finger 1st finger bilateral 1st finger sig move 1st point point finger join tips of fingers finger of finger join tips of fingers dominant hand along side of opposite 1st finger tab side of in front in front or l; r 1st finger of opposite hand opposite centre of body finger centre of body orient horizontal verticalvertical-unilateral opposite vertical-palm away palm down unilateral from body opposite face the use of signs and the coding of prefix markers by teachers at a school foi the deaf ι myrtle l aron, ph d (witwatersrand·) i robyn ε lewis, ba(log.) (witwatersrand) ba soc.science (unisa) j judy l willemse, ba (sp. & η therapy) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract the use of aspects of an artificially devised manual code in a black schoolfor the deaf was examined. the encoding of prefixes, bound with the noun class system, in tswana as used by seven teachers was studied as well as the consistency of the teachers to code lexical items. results .indicated the absence of signed prefix markers, inconsistency in signing lexical items and much variability among teachers in the signs used. the educational and research implications are discussed. /'' opsomming die gebruik van aspekte van "n kunsmatig ontwikkelde gebarestelsel in 'n swart skool vir dowes, is ondersoek. die enkodering van voorvoegsels verbonde aan die naamwoordklasstelsel in tswana soos gebruik deur sewe onderwysers is bestudeer, asook die konstantheid van die onderwyser se vermoe om leksikale items te kodeer. resultate dui op die afwesigheid van voorvoegselgebare, onkonstantheid van leksikale gebare en baie variasie onder onderwysers t. o. v. die gebare wat hulle gebruik. die opvoedkundige en navorsingsimplikasies word bespreek. ^ ο sasha 1986 77^ south african journal of communication disorders, vol 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 7 gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens van hulpklasleerlinge teruggeplaas in hoofstroomonderwys karen kleingeld, brenda louw en isabel c. uys departement kommunikasiepatologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming resente navorsing le toenemend klem op die rol van die spraak-taalterapeut by taalleergestremde hinders 'n ^^uegter • γ tn^z satuur eeidentifiseer betreffende die taalfunksionering van die ouer taalleergestremde kind in die skool. b ^ z i i ^ t l t ^ t z n van gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme by leerlinge wat uit die hulpklas ter^geplaas ilnztroomonderwys, het as motivering vir hierdie studiegedien. die empinese studie is opagt-en-twintigafrikaa^en en^elsprekende stankerd twee oud-hulpklasleerlinge uitgevoer. die proefpersone se peil van taalen skolastiese l s s t s kwalitatief en kwantitatief ontleed en beskryf, dear die gebruik van die toets vir mondehnge taalproduksie r^die suidam.kaanse skryftaaltoets. die resultate van die studie toon dat 'n beduidende persentasie van die proefpersone z^^^zttas^obume ondervind en 'n agterstand in die vakke afrikaans en engels vertoon. betekenisvolle korrelasiesiscmn^toon tussen die proefpersone se gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens en hulle taalvermoens en .ftofarte» prestasies d^bevindinge hou belangrike implikasies in vir die bantering van die taalleergestremde kind m die praktyk en zox voortgesette spraak-taalterapeutiese intervensie aan by hinders wat uit die hulpklas teruggeplaas is η zofstroomonderwys. verdere navorsing oor die taalvermoens van die ouer taalleergestremde kind word aanbeveel ter uitbreiding en bevestiging van die navorsingsresultate. abstract current literature emphasizes the role of the speech-language therapist with regard » l " ^ * ? ™ * gabled children a lack of research regarding the language abilities of the older language learning disabled child was however identified the possmjity of continued spoken and written language problems in aid class children who have been placed back into mair^tream edulion, served as a motivation for this research project the on twenty-eight afrikaans arid english speaking ex-aid class children. according to experiment performlein spoken and written language and scholastic skills were analysed and described tively, through the test for θ\αι language production and the south african written language that a significant percentage of the subjects evidenced spoken and written language problems and the school subjects afrikaans and english. meaningful correlations were found to exist between guage, and language skills dnd scholastic performance. implications for dealing with the language learning disabkdchdd ire discussed, l!continuation of speech-language intervention with aid class c^^n ^o w e ^ mainstream education, is strongly recommended. a need for further research with regard to the older language learning disabled child is expressed in order to support and expand these results. sleutelwoorde: taalleergestremde kinder s, hulpklas, gesprokeen geskrewe taalprobleme, geheeltaalbenadering, samewerkende dienslewering. inleiding die spraak-taalterapeut se rol in hulpverlening aan kinders met leerprobleme het toegeneem in samehang met navorsing oor die verband tussen taalen leerprobleme (hill & haynes, 1992). taal blyk onontbeerlik te wees vir die leerproses in die skool, met die gevolg dat taalprobleme aanleiding gee tot die ontwikkeling van leerprobleme (wallach, 1990). in die lig van bogenoemde behoort die spraak-taalterapeut dus as aktiewe lid van die multidissiplinere span op te tree in die voorkoming, identifisering en remediering van onderliggende taalprobleme by die taalleergestremde p'opulasie. leerlinge met ernstige leerprobleme word dikwels in die hulpklas, 'n aparte remedierende klas binne skoolverband, geplaas (haines, 1988). die doel van die hulpklas is om aan die kind met leerprobleme intensiewe remedierende hulp te verleen en akademiese vordering op 'n individuele grondslag te monitor. hulpverlening vind in spanverband plaas en die onderwyser, opvoedkundige sielkundige, spraak-taalterapeut, arbeidsterapeut en mediese dokter lewer insette om die skolastiese, taal, perseptuele en fisiese probleme wat saamhang met die hulpklasleerling se leerprobleem, te ondervang (itempelhoff, 1994). resente navorsing oor die taalvermoens van afrikaanssprekende hulpklasleerlinge toon aan d&t tot soveel as 94% van hierdie leerlinge geringe tot die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 karen kleingeld, brenda louw & isabel c. uys ernstige orale semantiese afwykings vertoon, wat negatief op skolastiese'prestasie kan inwerk (du plessis, 1992). dit is dus duidelik dat die spraak-taalterapeut wat in die skole werksaam is, aandag moet skenk aan taalprobleme wat by hulpklasleerlinge voorkom. hulpklasleerlinge kan gespesialiseerde onderrig egter slegs tot op negejarige ouderdom ontvang, waarna hulle teruggeplaas moet word in hoofstroomonderwys, ongeag of hulle leerprobleme opgelos is of nie (derbyshire, 1989). die verband wat uitgewys is tussen taal en leer vero'nderstel dat taalprobleme steeds by die nie-gerehabiliteerde oud-hulpklasleerlinge kan voorkom. opvolgstudies oor die verloop van taalprobleme bewys ook dat taalprobleme met tyd verander, maar bly voortbestaan (hill & haynes, 1992). aangesien onderliggende en onopgeloste taalprobleme dus steeds akademiese vordering kan beperk, kan die taak van die spraak-taalterapeut nie as afgehandel beskou word sodra die taalleergestremde kind gereed is om weer in die hoofstroom teruggeplaas te word nie. 'n leemte wat egter in die literatuur en in die praktyk gei'dentifiseer is, is navorsingsbevindinge met be trekking tot die spesifieke taalvermoens van leerlinge wat hulpklasonderrig ontvang het. omdat die taalleergestremde leerling wat in 'n hulpklas was aan 'n unieke hulpverleningsituasie blootgestel is, kan bestaande literatuur oor die taalvermoens van taalleergestremde leerlinge nie sonder meer op die oud-hulpklasleerling toegepas word nie. die implikasies wat die voortbestaan van gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme by oud-hulpklasleerlinge vir spraak-taalterapeutiese dienslewering inhou, noodsaak dus verdere navorsing in hierdie verband. tbenemende klem wat tans in die onderwys op 'n voorkomende, eerder as 'n remedierende benadering geplaas word (williams, 1995), identifiseer 'n behoefte aan navorsing wat aanleiding kan gee tot voorkomende optrede in spraaktaalintervensie. verder fokus resente onderwysgerigte navorsing in suid-afrika op die insluiting van kinders met spesiale behoeftes in hoofstroomonderwys (naidoo, burden, topham & singh, 1996), wat die belang van spraaktaalterapeutiese navorsing by hoofstroom taalleergestremde kinders beklemtoon. aangesien 'n geheeltaalbenadering tot taalintervensie by leergestremde kinders voorgehou word, en geskrewe en gesproke taal as 'n eenheid binne hierdie benadering figureer (smith-burke, deegan & jaggar, 1991), stel die empiriese studie dit ten doel om die navorsingsleemte wat gei'dentifiseer is, binne die raamwerk van geskrewe en gesproke taal te ondersoek. resente navorsing oor die verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal by leergestremde kinders steun die belang van verdere navorsing oor spesifiek gesproke en geskrewe taal, ten einde onopgeloste vraagstukke wat steeds in die literatuur voorkom, aan te spreek (oosthuizen, 1994). metode doel die doel van die studie is om die gesproke en geskrewe taalfunksionering van 'n groep leerlinge wat hulpklasonderrig ontvang het, te beskryf deur: die aard van gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens en skolastiese prestasies by die proefpersone te bepaal. die proefpersone se gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens, asook taalvermoens en skolastiese funksionering te vergelyk. navorsingsontwerp die komplekse aard van gesproke en geskrewe taal, wat vir die doel van hierdie studie ondersoek is, leen hom tot die gebruik van 'n gekombineerde kwantitatiewekwalitatiewe ontwerp. 'n opname metode is as navorsingsontwerp gebruik, en kwantitatiewe (statistiese) en tabel 1. opsommende kenmerke van proefpersone kenmerke van proefpersone subgroepe aantal en % proefpersone (n = 28) moedertaal afrikaans engels 14 (50%) 14 (50%) ouderdom (jare-maande) 9-0 tot 9-11 10-0 tot 10-11 11-0 tot 11-11 3 (11%) 18 (64%) 7 (25%) geslag manlik vroulik 19 (68%) 9 (32%) intelligensiekwosient (osais) 9 5 9 9 100 109 110 119 120 en hoer 4 (14%) 14 (50%) 8 (29%) 2 (7%) geskiedenis van otitis media geen eenmalige episode herhaaldelik 19 (68%) 2 (7%) 7 (25%) aandag-eh konsentrasieprobleme . geen aandagafleibaarheid 19 (68%) 9 (32%) arbeidsterapie geen terapie ontvang terapie ontvang 21(75%) 7 (25%) spraak-taalterapie geen terapie ontvang terapie ontvang 20 (71%) 8 .(29%) ekstra remedierende onderrig geen ekstra onderrig ekstra onderrig ontvang 17 (61%) 11 (39%) tydsduur in hulpklas minder as 6 maande een jaar twee jaar 1 (4%) 18(64%) 9 (32%) rede vir uitplasing uit hulpklas opheffing van leerprobleem maksimum ouderdom 22 (79%) 6 (21%) tydsverloop sedert terugplasing in hoofstroom een jaar twee jaar drie jaar 8 (28%) 19 (68%) 1 (4%) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens van hulpklasleerlinge teruggeplaas in hoofstroomonderwys kwalitatiewe (beskrywende) analises van verkree data is aanvullend tot mekaar uitgevoer (leedy, 1 9 9 3 ) . resultate proefpersoonseleksie agt en twintig standerd twee leerlinge wat voorheen hulpklasonderrig ontvang het, is as proefpersoonpopulasie geselekteer uit staatsondersteunde afrikaanse en engelse laerskole in benoni en kempton park. kwotasteekproefneming (leedy, 1993) is gebruik in die verkryging van 'n proefpersoon-populasie van ongeveer 30 leerlinge, waarvan 50% deur afrikaanssprekende leerlinge en 50% deur engelssprekende leerlinge verteenwoordig moes word. opsommende kenmerke van die geselekteerde proefpersone word in tabel 1 weergegee. data-insameling die proefpersone is individueel deur middel van gestandaardiseerde gesproke (tmt en tolp) en geskrewe taaltoetse (sast en sawlt) deur die betrokke navorser geevalueer. die toetse is volgens voorgeskrewe standaard prosedures in die toetshandleidings op die proefpersone uitgevoer. by afrikaanssprekende leerlinge is die tmt (vorster, 1980a) en die sast (brink, 1976a) afgeneem, en by engelssprekende leerlinge die tolp (vorster, 1980b) en die sawlt (brink, 1976b). toetsresultate is volgens riglyne gestel deur die handleidings van die tmt/tolp en die sast/sawlt te ontleed. vraelyste oor huidige taalen skolastiese funksionering van die proefpersone is aan die klasonderwysers van die betrokke leerlinge verskaf. vraelyste oor die agtergrondsgeskiedenis van elke proefpersoon is aan die betrokke ouers oorgedra. die proefpersone se finale standerd-twee punte, asook alle ander relevante inligting is uit kumulatiewe verslagkaarte en skolastiese rekords versamel. dataverwerking dataverwerkingsprosedures wat uitgevoer is om elkeen van die doelstellings van die studie te bereik word in tabel 2 saamgevat. tabel 2. dataverwerking van toetsresultate die aard van proefpersone se gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens en skolastiese vordering beskrywing van die proefpersone se gesproke taalvermoens uit die bestudering van die proefpersone se gesproke taalvermoens volgens die tmt en tolp word die gevolgtrekking gemaak dat proefpersone hoofsaaklik ondergemiddeld presteer in totale woorde, tipe-teken ratio, bywoorde, medewerkwoorde, sinstruktuur-verbeterings, woordkeuse-verbeterings, woordherhalings en abstrak-konkreetheidsindeks, en dat hierdie swak prestasies aanleiding gee tot 'n hoe persentasie ondergemiddelde prestasies in die vlotheid(50%), produktiwiteit(38,1%) en kompleksiteitskale (39,2%) (sien p r o d u k l j w i l a l t k a r r a k t h e l d i n h o u d k o m p i e k i r t f l i l v l o l h e l d t o t a a l n a s i e n s k a l e v a n t m t e n t o l p i % o n d e r g e m i d d e l d % g e m l d d e l d * b o g e m l d d e l d figuur 1: ondergemiddelde, gemiddelde en bogemiddelde prestasies van proefpersone in mondelinge taalproduksie 1 doelstellings 1 roudata statistiese verwerkings voorstelling van resultate 1 aard van gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens en skolastiese funksionering * staneges tmt (vorster, 1980a) en tolp (vorster, /1980b) * staneges sast (brink, 1976a) en sawlt (brink, 1976b) * ordinale data vraelyste en skolastiese rekords * geweegde rekenkundige gemiddeldes * frekwensies * persentasies (leedy, 1993) * figure 1 tot 3 frekwensie verspreidingsdiagramme staafdiagramme 2 verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal en tussen taalvermoens en skolastiese prestasie * staneges tmt (vorster, 1980a) en tolp (vorster, 1980b) * staneges sast (brink, 1976a) en sawlt (brink, 1976b) * ordinale data vraelyste en skolastiese rekords: omgeskakel in kwantitatiewe vorm * spearman korrelasiekoeffisient vasstelling van betekenisvolle positiewe of negatiewe korrelasies tussen data (leedy, 1993) * tabel 3 * tabel 4 die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 figuur 1). dit blyk dus dat die proefpersone ten spyte van hulpklasonderrig steeds gesproke taalprobleme ondervmd wat verband hou met die lengte en kompleksiteit van huue uitinge, en dat hulle gesproke taal gekenmerk word deur mondelinge verbeterings wat vlotheid b^nvloed. m d bevindinge word ondersteun deur de koker (1981) se ondersoek na taalprobleme by leergestremde kinders. beskrywing van die proefpersone se geskrewe taalvermoens uit die beskrywing van die proefpersone se geskrewe taalfunksionering word die gevolgtrekking gemaak dat 27,4% van die proefpersone ondergemiddeld presteer in geskrewe taal volgens die sast en sawlt. in sinskompleksiteit en korrektheid het onderskeidelik 21,4% en 28,6% van die proefpersone swak presteer, maar die lae persentasie goeie prestasies (10,7%) wat in hierdie subtoetse behaal is, verklaar waarskynlik die waarneming dat sinskompleksiteit en korrektheid die swakste vertoon het in die gemiddelde stanege waardes wat vir elke subtoets vasgestel is. onderskeidelik 32,1%, 42,9% en 35,7% van die proefpersone het swak presteer in totale woorde, totale sinne en woorde per sin, wat op 'n algemene agterstand in hierdie geskrewe taalvaardighede dui (sien figuur 2). navorsing ondersteun die voorkoms van geskrewe taalprobleme, soos lae kompleksiteit en korrektheid, by taalleergestremde kinders (carlisle, 1994). samevattend dui die resultate oor die aard van die proefpersone se taalvaardighede daarop dat 'n beduidende persentasie van die proefpersone ondergemiddelde gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens vertoon en dat meer proefpersone probleme ondervind in gesproke taalvermoens (33,2%) as in geskrewe taalvermoens (27,4%). gesproke taalfoute kan hoofsaaklik toegeskryf word aan ondergemiddelde tellings in vlotheid, produktiwiteit en kompleksiteit, terwyl beperkte sinskompleksiteit en korrektheid verantwoordelik was vir swak prestasies van die proefpersone in geskrewe taal. beskrywing van die proefpersone se skolastiese vordering vraelyste wat deur die onderwysers ingevul is dui daarop dat die proefpersone as 'n groep gemiddeld in die klaskamer funksioneer. spellingprobleme verteenwoordig die grootste karen kleingeld, brenda louw & isabel c. uys persentasie swak prestasies (35,7%) en probleme in wiskunde en geskrewe taal kom by onderskeidelik 25% en 26,2% van die proefpersone voor. in die geval van wiskunde het 'n groter persentasie van die proefpersone (26,8%) egter goeie prestasies vertoon. lees, algemene akademiese vaardighede (bv. vermoe om selfstandig te werk), mondelinge taal en algemene kommunikasie-vaardighede (bv. beantwoording van vrae) blyk die minste probleme by die proefpersone te veroorsaak (sien figuur 3). in die geheel gesien presteer die proefpersone dus gemiddeld in skolastiese vordering. 42,9% tot 46,4% van die proefpersone se prestasies in die vakke afrikaans en engels vertoon egter veral in die areas van spelling en skryfvermoens, byvoorbeeld korrektheid en kompleksiteit van sinne, 'n agterstand. hierdie bevindinge word deur bestaande navorsing ondersteun (stackhouse, 1992). vergelyking van die proefpersone se gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens, asook taalvermoens en skolastiese vordering vergelyking van die proefpersone se gesproke en geskrewe taalfunksionering in tabel 3 word die subtoetse van die tmt en tolp en die sast en sawlt wat betekenisvolle verbande met mekaar vertoon, uiteengesit. 'n positiewe korrelasie is geidentifiseer in die vergelyking van die proefpersone se abstrak-konkreetheidsindeks in gesproke taal en die abstrak-konkrete inhoudskaal in geskrewe taal, asook tussen gemiddelde staneges in gesproke taal en gemiddelde staneges in geskrewe taal. 'n negatiewe korrelasie tussen woorde per t-eenheid in gesproke taal en woorde per sin in geskrewe taal is by die proefpersone aangetoon. die afleiding word gemaak dat daar 'n verband bestaan tussen die proefpersone se prestasies in gesproke en geskrewe taal in terme van abstrakte taalinhoud, en ook in terme van totale gesproke en geskrewe taalprestasie, soos bevestig deur catts (1991). vergelyking van die proefpersone se gesproke en geskrewe taalfunksionering met skolastiese vordering 'n uiteensetting van die gesproke en geskrewe taalprestasies van proefpersone wat betekenisvolle verbande met skolastiese prestasies vertoon, word in tabel 4 verskaf. t o t a l s w o o r d e w o o r d e p e r s i n k o r r e k t h e i d t o t a a l t o t a l e s i n n e s i n s k o m p l e k s i t e i t a b s t r a k k o n k r e t e i n h o u d s a s t s u b t o e t s e t o t a l e w o o r d e w o o r d e p e r s i n k o r r e k t h e i d t o t a l e s i n n e s i n s k o m p l e k s i t e i t a b s t r a k k o n k r e t e ir s a s t s u b t o e t s e [ ϊ ϋ % s w a k • • "it g e m i d d e l d % g o o d figuur 2: swak, gemiddelde en goeie prestasies van proefpersone in geskrewe taal figuur 3: prestasies van proefpersone in algemene skolastiese vaardighede the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens van hulpklasleerlinge teruggeplaas in hoofstroomonderwys 11 subtoetse en nasienskale spearman korrelasiekoeffisient oorskrydingswaarskynlikheid (p-waarde) aard van korrelasie (op 10% peil van betekenis) gesproke taal tmt (vorster, 1980a); tolp (vorster, 1980b) geskrewe taal sast (brink, 1976a; sawlt (brink, 1976b) spearman korrelasiekoeffisient oorskrydingswaarskynlikheid (p-waarde) aard van korrelasie (op 10% peil van betekenis) totale aantal woorde totale woorde 0,154 0,212 geen betekenisvolle verwantskap woorde per t-eenheid woorde per sin -0,255 0,093 negatiewe verwantskap kompleksiteit-skaal kompleksiteit-skaal -0,054 0,39 geen betekenisvolle verwantskap korrektheidskaal korrektheidskaal -0,063 0,37 geen betekenisvolle verwantskap abstrak-konkreetheids-indeks abstrak-konkrete inhoudskaal 0,455 0,009 positiewe verwantskap gemiddelde staneges gemiddelde staneges 0,305 0,0571 positiewe verwantskap tabel 4: die aard van betekenisvolle korrelasies tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal funksionering by die proefpersone gesproke taal en skolastiese prestasie spearman korrelasiekoeffisient oorskrydingswaarskynlikheid (p-waarde) aard van korrelasie (op 10% peil van betekenis) gesproke taal en afrikaans 0,000 geen korrelasie gesproke taal en engels -0,500 0,24 negatiewe korrelasie gesproke taal en wiskunde 0,000 geen korrelasie gesproke taal en leervakke 0,500 0,24 geen korrelasie gesproke taal en lees 0,500 0,24 geen korrelasie gesproke taal en spelling 1,000 0,08 positiewe korrelasie gesproke taal en algemene akademiese funksionering' 0,500 0,24 geen korrelasie gesproke taal en algemene kommunikasiegedrag -0,500 0,24 negatiewe korrelasie gesproke taal en st 2 skolastiese vordering 1,000 0,08 positiewe korrelasie geskrewe taal en j skolastiese i prestasie : spearman korrelasiekoeffisient oorskrydingswaarskynlikheid (p-waarde) aard van korrelasie (op 10% peil van betekenis) geskrewe taal en afrikaans 0,000 geen korrelasie geskrewe taal en engels 0,500 0,24 negatiewe korrelasie geskrewe taal en wiskunde 0,000 geen korrelasie geskrewe taal en leervakke 0,500 0,24 geen korrelasie geskrewe taal en lees 0,500 0,24 geen korrelasie geskrewe taal en spelling 1,000 0,08 positiewe korrelasie geskrewe taal en algemene akademiese funksionering 0,500 0,24 geen korrelasie geskrewe taal en algemene kommunikasiegedrag -0,500 0,24 negatiewe korrelasie geskrewe taal en st 2 skolastiese vordering 1 1,000 0,08 positiewe korrelasie die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 karen kleingeld, brenda louw & isabel c. uys volgens die resultate kom beide positiewe en negatiewe korrelasies tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal en skolastiese prestasie by die proefpersone voor. die proefpersone se taalvermoens toon 'n positiewe verband met hul st 2 skolastiese vordering (hill & haynes, 1992) en spelling (lerner, 1993) en 'n negatiewe verband met prestasie in die skoolvak engels en algemene kommunikasiegedrag. bespreking 'n opsommende weergawe van die eksperimentele doelstellings en verbandhoudende resultate word in figuur 4 verskaf, en dien as raamwerk vir die bespreking van die navorsingsresultate. bespreking van resultate oor die aard van gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens en skolastiese vordering uit die beskrywing van die resultate is dit duidelik dat die proefpersone uitvalle vertoon in gesproke taalvermoens. verskeie outeurs bevestig dat taalleergestremde kinders swakker gesproke taalvermoens vertoon as normaalpresterende kinders (lerner, 1993; klein & harris, 1986). wat die aard van orale taalproblematiek betref, ondersteun die huidige studie navorsingsbevindinge van de koker (1981), dat onderprestasies in alle subtoetse van die tmt voorgekom het by taalleergestremde kinders. die resultate van die proefpersone se gesproke taalvermoens dui daarop dat die meeste ontoepaslike gedrag voorgekom het met betrekking tot produktiwiteit en vlotheid van mondelinge taalproduksie. navorsers het ter ondersteuning hiervan bevind dat die gemiddelde lengte van iaitinge by taalleergestremde kinders opvallend laer is as die van nie-leergestremde kinders (oosthuizen, 1994) en dat die taalleergestremde kind minder doelmatige woorde in spontane verbale ekspressie produseer, wat dus produktiwiteit verlaag (derbyshire, 1989). welman (1986) bevestig die voorkoms van onvlotheid as een van die taalleergestremde kind se grootste probleme. die resultate van die ondersoek na die proefpersone se gesproke taalvermoens, toon dus dat 'n agterstand in gesproke taalvermoens ten spyte van hulpklasonderrig, steeds by 'n beduidende persentasie van die proefpersone voorkom en dus langdurig van aard is. aram, enkelman & nation (1984) het in ooreenstemming met hierdie resultate bevind dat gesproke taalprobleme wat voorskools gei'dentifiseer is, kan voortduur tot in adolessensie. geskrewe taal prestasies dui daarop dat die proefpersone oorwegend op 'n laag gemiddelde vlak funksioneer en dat 46,4% van die proefpersone waarskynlik aangewese is op ekstra hulpverlening vir die verbetering van geskrewe taalvermoens. hierdie resultate stem ooreen met resente literatuurbewyse wat die bestaan van geskrewe taalprobleme by taalleergestremde kinders ondersteun (carlisle, 1994). schumaker & deshler (1984) het bevind dat die probleme wat taalleergestremde kinders in geskrewe uitdrukkingsvermoens ervaar, tot in adolessensie voortduur. die huidige studieresultate bevestig dus vorige bevindinge oor die voorkoms van geskrewe taalprobleme by ouer taalleergestremde kinders. die empiriese studie toon verder dat 33,5% van die totale aantal foute wat deur proefpersone in die korrektheidskaal van die sast en sawlt gemaak is, direk toe te skryf is aan swak woordgebruik en foute in sinskonstruksie. lerner (1993) erken die voorkoms van woordvindingsprobleme in taalleergestremde kinders se geskrewe taal. moran (1988) wys egter daarop dat sintaktiese foute by taalleergestremde kinders eerder verband hou met aandagtekorte as met 'n beperkte kennis van sintaksis-reels. beperkte geskrewe taalkorrektheid kom waarskynlik ook voor opidat taalleergestremde kinders dikwels nie geskrewe take proeflees vir spelfoute en punktuasiefoute nie (buttrill, niizawa, biemer, takahashi & hearn, 1989). verskeie navorsers bewys die voorkoms van spelfoute en punktuasiefoute in die geskrewe taal van taalleergestremde kinders en bevestig dus die resultate van die huidige studie (hallahan & kauffman, 1991; gearheart & gearheart, 1989). dit blyk dus dat die bestaan van ekspressief geskrewe taalprobleme by die proefpersone nie ontken kan word nie, maar dat hierdie probleme nie opvallend by alle oud-hulpklasleerlinge wat as proefpersone gedien het, voorkom nie. die beskrywing van die studie-resulfate dui aan dat die proefpersone uitvalle in skolastiese funksionering vertoon en dat slegs 23% van die proefpersone daarin kon slaag om bo die standerdgemiddeld vir standerd-twee te presteer terwyl 38% van die proefpersone onder^die standerdgemiddeld presteer het. hierdie bevinding stem ooreen met literatuurbewyse oor die voorkoms van skolastiese probleme by taalleergestremde kinders (hill & haynes, 1992). die bevindinge impliseer verder dat skolastiese probleme wat by die proefpersone voorgekom het, nie in die hulpklas opgehef is nie, en dat addisionele rejnediering benodig word. beskrywing van die aard van skolastiese probleme wat deur die proefpersone ondervind word, dui daarop dat ongeveer 50% van die proefpersone ondergemiddeld presteer het in die skoolvakke afrikaans en engels. hierdie bevinding hou waarskynlik verband met die gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme wat reeds by die proefpersone gei'dentifiseer is, aangesien prestasies in die skoolvakke afrikaans en engels hoofsaaklik op die evaluering van mondelinge, spelling, lees en opstelle berus (derbyshire, 1989). die resultate verkry uit die onderwysersvraelyste bevestig hierdie veronderstelling en toon aan dat spelling en ander geskrewe taalprobleme die grootste bydrae lewer tot ondergemiddelde skolastiese prestasies by die proefpersone. literatuur ondersteun die hoe voorkoms van skolastiese probleme, wat verband hou met geskrewe taal, by taalleergestremde kinders (stackhouse, 1992). 'n beduidende persentasie van die proefpersone behoort dus steeds in die areas van spelling en skryfvermoens in die vakke afrikaans en engels hulp te ontvang, omdat hierdie vaardighede en vakke 'n sentrale deel vorm van akademiese werk in sowel die primere as doelstelling 1 gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme en skolastiese probleme | gesproke taal | geskrewe taal | | skolastiese funksionering ] • kompleksiteit • sinkompleksiteit • skoollakke: air, eng, wisk • vlotheid • korrektheid • spelling • produktiwiteit • geskrewe taal doelstelling 2 korrelasie tussen gesproke e n geskrewe taal, en taal en skolastiese funksionering jgesproke e n geskrewe taal | | taal e n skolastiese funksionring j • abstrakte taalinhoud · taal e n spelling • totale gesproke e n geskrewe taal · taal e n vordering in standerd 2 figuur 4: 'n skematiese voorstelling van die resultate the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) geskrewe taalvermoens van hulpklasleerlinge teruggeplaas in hoofstroomonderwys 13 die sekondere skoolfases. ondergemiddelde prestasies wat in wiskunde voorgekom het, stem ooreen met navorsingsbevindinge oor die voorkoms van wiskunde-probleme by taalleergestremde kinders (haylock, 1992). literatuur dui ter verdere ondersteuning van die studieresultate aan dat wiskunde-probleme wat deur taalleergestremde kinders ondervind word, by ouer kinders voortduur (maxwell & wallach, 1984). die afleiding word gemaak dat die leerprobleme wat deur die proefpersone ervaar is, van so 'n aard was dat dit nie ten voile deur hulpklasonderrig uitgeskakel kon word nie en dat hierdie kinders dus aangewese is op verdere hulpverlening op skolastiese gebied na uitplasing uit die hulpklas. samevattend dui die resultate van doelstelling 1 van die empiriese studie op die volgehoue aard van gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme, sowel as beperkinge in skolastiese funksionering by 'n beduidende persentasie van die proefpersone. bespreking en verklaring van korrelasies tussen gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens, en taalvermoens en skolastiese vordering resente literatuur ondersteun 'n geheeltaalbenadering tot geletterdheidsontwikkeling en lig so die verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal uit (smith-burke et al., 1991). volgens die empiriese ondersoek is 'n positiewe korrelasie tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal by die proefpersone gei'dentifiseer wanneer abstrak-konkreetheidsindeks in gesproke taal en abstrak-konkrete inhoudskaal in geskrewe taal, vergelyk word. in die bespreking van gesproke en geskrewe taal-toetsresultate is opgemerk dat die proefpersone oor die algemeen gemiddeld presteer het in gesproke en geskrewe abstrakte taalgebruik. hierdie bevinding is teenstrydig met literatuurbevindinge wat die voorkoms van abstrakte taalprobleme by taalleergestremde kinders beklemtoon (lerner, 1993). ongeag van die aard van die huidige proefpersone se abstrakte taalvermoens, blyk dit egter uit die resultate dat gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens,' wat abstrakte taalgebruik aanbetref, verband hou. hierdie verband impliseer dat proefpersone wat wel gesproke abstrakte taalprobleme ondervind, waarskynlik ook geskrewe abstrakte taalprobleme sal vertoon, wat laangespreek moet word in s p r a a k t a a l t e r a p e u t i e s e intervensie ten einde die ontwikkeling van gepaardgaande skolastiese probleme te verminder. 'n positiewe korrelasie is ook tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal vasgestel wanneer gemiddelde staneges in gesproke taal en gemiddelde staneges in geskrewe taal vergelyk word. die veronderstelling word dus gemaak dat kinders met gesproke taalprobleme waarskynlik ook geskrewe taalprobleme sal ondervind en dat 'n verbetering in gesproke taal aanleiding sal gee tot 'n verbetering in geskrewe taal. verskeie navorsers ondersteun die voorkoms van 'n verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal (oosthuizen, 1994; catts, 1991). volgens maxwell & wallach (1984) word veranderinge in taalsimptome oor tyd veral sigbaar wanneer vroee gesproke taalprobleme in geskrewe taalprobleme gemanifesteer word. onderwysers is egter nie altyd bewus van die verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal nie, en skenk dikwels aandag aan die geskrewe taalprobleem sonder om ook die verbandhoudende gesproke taalprobleme aan te spreek (gearheart & gearheart, 1989). die resultate van die empiriese studie beklemtoon egter die belang van 'n geheeltaalbenadering in die behandeling van gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme wat by die proefpersone gei'dentifiseer is. 'n negatiewe korrelasie is in die huidige studie tussen gesproke taal en geskrewe taal aangetoon wanneer woorde per t-eenheid in gesproke taal en woorde per sin in geskrewe taal, vergelyk word. horowitz (1990) wys daarop dat gesproke taal minder gestruktureerd en kompleks as geskrewe taal is, en dat die taalgebruiker meer selfbewus is in die gebruik van geskrewe as gesproke taal. geskrewe taal is dus meer gekontroleerd en akademies, en moet meer sorgvuldig beplan word (owens, 1992). hierdie sienings ondersteun resultate van die huidige studie, wat daarop wys dat die proefpersone wat lang sinne in gesproke taal gebruik het, korter geskrewe taalsinne vertoon het. gillam & johnston (1992) bevestig dat taalleergestremde kinders korter sinne in geskrewe as in gesproke taal gebruik. die resultate van 'n vergelyking tussen die proefpersone se g e s p r o k e en g e s k r e w e t a a l v e r m o e n s b e v e s t i g dus literatuurbewyse wat ooreenkomste, sowel as verskille tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal ondersteun (wallach, 1990). soos reeds genoem, kom reseptiewe en ekspressiewe taalprobleme wat taalleergestremde kinders se skolastiese prestasies beperk, tot in adolessensie voor (buttrill et al., 1989). die resultate van die studie bevestig dat 'n wisselwerking tussen taalprobleme en leerprobleme voorkom. volgens die resultate van die empiriese ondersoek is korrelasies by die proefpersone tussen die sekere areas van taal en skolastiese funksionering gei'dentifiseer. 'n positiewe korrelasie is aangedui tussen die proefpersone se gesproke en geskrewe taal en hulle spelling. navorsing oor spellingprobleme by taalleergestremde kinders het 'n verband tussen die kinders se taalvermoens en hulle spelling bepaal (kriegler, du toit & smart, 1990). die voorkoms van spellingprobleme by die proefpersone hou dus waarskynlik verband met die geskrewe taalprobleme wat reeds by die proefpersone gei'dentifiseer is. carlisle (1994) skryf die voorkoms van spellingprobleme toe aan gebrekkige linguistiese kennis, en wys daarop dat spellingprobleme aanleiding gee tot geskrewe taalprobleme omdat die taalleergestremde kinders korter en meer eenvoudige stories as kinders sonder spellingprobleme skryf. om korrek te kan spel word van die skoolgaande kind vereis om oordele oor gesproke en geskrewe taal te kan maak. beperkinge in metalinguistiese vermoens word dus aangevoer as een van die redes waarom taalleergestremde kinders spellingprobleme ondervind (owens, 1992). verdere navorsing word benodig om die presiese verband tussen ontwikkelende linguistiese en metalinguistiese vermoens, en spelling-ontwikkeling te ondersoek (carlisle, 1994). positiewe korrelasies is verder gei'dentifiseer tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal en standerd-twee skolastiese vordering by die proefpersone. die kontinuum van taalleerprobleme wat deur maxwell & wallach (1984) voorgestel is, word deur die korrelasie bevestig. die verband tussen taalvermoens en skolastiese funksionering wat deur die resultate gei'dentifiseer is, word algemeen in die literatuur aanvaar (hill & haynes, 1992). die korrelasies wat na aanleiding van doelstelling twee vasgestel is, bevestig dus 'n algemene verband tussen taalvermoens en skolastiese prestasie, ten spyte van die feit dat spesifieke skolastiese vaardighede nie 'n reglynige verband met gesproke en geskrewe taalfunksionering vertoon nie. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 gevolgtrekkings en aanbevelings die identifisering van gesproke taalprobleme by die proefpersone, en die verband wat in die literatuur uitgewys word tussen onderliggende gesproke taalprobleme en leesen skryfprobleme (gillon & dodd, 1995) impliseer dat verdere spraak-taalterapeutiese intervensie by oud-hulpklasleerlinge van die uiterste belang is. insluiting van hierdie kinders by die reeds oorvol program van die spraaktaalterapeut wat in die skole werksaam is, stel egter hoer eise wat werkslading aanbetref. die spraak-taalterapeut word dus genoodsaak om alternatiewe diensleweringsmodelle in te skakel by die evaluering en behandeling van ouer taalleergestremde kinders (wright, 1992). deur middel van samewerkende dienslewering (creaghead, 1994) moet die spraak-taalterapeut poog om deur samewerking met die ouers, onderwysers en ander professionele persone effektiewe intervensie te bewerkstellig by leerlinge wat uit die hulpklas teruggeplaas is in hoofstroomonderwys. gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme wat in hierdie studie gei'dentifiseer is, behoort in 'n geheeltaalbenadering deur die spraak-taalterapeut aangespreek te word. deur middel van die geheeltaalbenadering maak die gefragmenteerde aard van afsonderlike hulpverlening vir gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme, plek vir integrasie van die taalkomponente, om oordrag te vergemaklik en samewerking tussen intervensie-spanlede aan te moedig (norris & hoffman, 1993). spraak-taalterapeute is egter tans nie ten voile opgelei om lees, spelling, wiskunde of kreatiewe skryf te hanteer, en dus die taalleergestremde kind as geheel te beskou nie (oberstein, 1990). opleiding van die spraak-taalterapeut behoort verbreed te word om meer in-diepte opleiding oor geskrewe taalintervensie in te sluit. aansluitend hierby stel uys (1993) voor dat reeds gekwalifiseerde spraak-taalterapeute konferensies, werkwinkels en simposiums, maar ook modules van nuwe voorgraadse kursusse sal bywoon, om met nuwe raamwerke bekend te raak. uitbreiding van die spraak-taalterapeut se vaardighede op die gebied van geskrewe taalvermoens, sal hom/haar in staat stel om as aktiewe lid van die multi-professionele span wat taalleergestremde kinders in die skoolkonteks hanteer, te funksioneer. die spraak-taalterapeut kan deur middel van spansamewerking ook opvoedkundige personeel se bewustheid van die rol van die spraak-taalterapeut in geskrewe taalprobleme, verhoog (sanger, hux & gries, 1995). die interverwantskap wat tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van die proefpersone uitgewys is, impliseer die belang van gesproke taal in geletterdheidsontwikkeling, en ondersteun dus vroee intervensie by kinders wat risikofaktore vertoon vir die ontwikkeling van spraaken taalprobleme (fletcher & foorman, 1994). vroegtydige bewusmaking van ouers en onderwysers van die ingrypende invloed van taalprobleme op skolastiese vordering, kan die stimulasie van voorgeletterdheidsvaardighede aanmoedig (schuele & van kleeck, 1987), wat die risiko vir die ontwikkeling van sekondere taalleerprobleme soos leesen skryfprobleme kan verlaag (scarborough & dobrich, 1990). 'n opvolgstudie oor die taalen skolastiese funksionering van oud-hulpklasleerlinge in die hoerskool, word in die lig van die resultate van die huidige studie en ter uitbreiding van beperkte literatuur oor die oud-hulpklaskaren kleingeld, brenda louw & isabel c. uys leerling in die hoerskool, aanbeveel (haines, 1988). ter aansluiting behoort die effektiwiteit van implementering van 'n samewerkende diensleweringsmodel vir opvolg en intervensie by die oud-hulpklasleerling en ouer taalleergestremde kind, ondersoek te word. na aanleiding van die groot persentasie wat taalleergestremde kinders van die spraak-taalterapeut se gevalsbelading uitmaak, word die uitvoering van kliniese navorsing met taalleergestremde kinders aanbeveel. aksie-navorsing sal die spraak-taalterapeut in staat stel om intervensieprosedures wat gevolg word met taalleergestremde kinders, voortdurend te evalueer en aan te pas by nuwe navorsingsbevindinge (wallach, 1990). behoeftes wat in die praktyk bestaan aan wetenskaplik verantwoordbare programme vir spraak-taalterapeutiese intervensie by taalleergestremde kinders, sal ook op hierdie wyse aangespreek kan word. die empiriese studie is van waarde vir die kliniese praktyk, omdat dit die basis daarstel vir opvolg-intervensie by die ouer taalleergestremde kind, asook vir verdere navorsing oor die taalen skolastiese funksionering van oud-hulpklasleerlinge. bevindinge oor die teenwoordigheid van gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme by proefpersone wat uit die hulpklas teruggeplaas is in hoofstroomonderwys, hou belangrike kliniese en navorsingsimplikasies vir die spraak-taalterapeut in. dit blyk uit die empiriese studie dat die spraak-taalterapeut beide gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens by taalleergestremde kinders behoort aan te spreek, ten einde effektiewe dienslewering moontlik te· maak, en die ontwikkeling van skolastiese probleme sover moontlik te beperk. toepaslike funksionering van die taalleergestremde kind in die klaskamersituasie sowel as in die sosiale konteks, word deur spraak-taalintervensie ten doel gestel. die rol van die spraak-taalterapeut in skole, asook die struktuur van hulpklasse, ondergaan tans ingrypende veranderinge om aan te pas by die nuwe suid-afrika konteks. die insluitings-beleid wat tans in die gauteng departement van onderwys voorgestel word (naidoo et al., 1996), impliseer dat hoofstroomleerlinge ook op tydelike basis by die hulpklas ingesluit moet word, sodat aandag aan spesifieke leerprobleme geskenk kan word. ter aansluiting by die gevolgtrekkings wat uit die huidige studie gemaak is, sal die spraak-taalterapeut se betrokke nheid by die taalleergestremde kind dus verder as die gei'dentifiseerde hulpklasleerling moet strek. die spraaktaalterapeut wat in die skole werksaam is, sal in die toekoms moet poog om alle taalleergestremde kinders deur middel van konsultasie met die hulpklasonderwyser, hoofstroomonderwysers en ouers (williams, 1995), by spraak-taalintervensie te betrek. erkenning hierdie artikel is gebaseer op die μ kommunikasiepatologie verhandeling k. kleingeld, gesproke en geskrewe taalvaardighede van kinders wat uit die hulpklas teruggeplaas is in hoofstroomonderwys, 1995, universiteit van pretoria, uitgevoer onder leiding van b. louw en i.c. uys. verwysings aram, d.m., ekelman, b.l. & nation, j.e. (1984). preschoolers with language disorders: 10 years later. journal of speech and hearing research, 27, 232-244. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens van hulpklasleerlinge teruggeplaas in hoofstroomonderwys brink m. (1976a). die suid-afrikaanse skryftaaltoets. pretoria: suid-afrikaanse raad vir geesteswetenskaplike navorsing. brink m. (1976b). the south african written language test. pretoria: south african human sciences research council. buttrill, j., niizawa, j., biemer, c., takahashi, c. & hearn, s. (1989). serving the language learning disabled adolescent: a strategies-based model. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 20, 170184. carlisle, j.f. (1994). morphological awareness, spelling, and story writing: possible relationships for elementary-age children with and without learning disabilities. in jordan, n.c. & goldsmith-phillips, j. (eds): learning disabilities : new directions for assessment and intervention. boston: allyn and bacon. catts h.w. (1991). early identification of reading disabilities. topics in language disorders, 12(1), 1-16. creaghead, n.a. (1994). collaborative intervention. in ripich, d.n. & creaghead, n.a. (eds): school discourse problems (2nd ed). san diego: singular publishing group, inc. de koker, e. (1981). 'n ondersoek na taalprobleme by leergestremde kinders. die suid-afrikaaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 28, 16-27. derbyshire, e.j (1989). leergestremdheid. in kapp, j.a. (red): kinders metprobleme 'n ortopedagogiese perspektief. pretoria: serva uitgewers. du plessis, j.m.j. (1992). die prevalensie en aard van orale semantiese afwykings byafrikaanse hulpklasleerlinge. m.log.verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria. fletcher, j.m. & foorman, b.r. (1994). issues in definition and measurement of learning disabilities the need for early intervention. in lyon, g.r. (ed): frames of reference for the assessment of learning disabilities new views on measurement issues. baltimore: paul h. brookes publishing co. gearheart, b.r. & gearheart, c.j. (1989). learning disabilities: educational strategies (5th ed.). columbus: merrill publishing company. gillon, g. & dodd, b. (1995). the effect of training phonological, semantic, and syntactic processing skills in spoken language on reading ability. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 26, 58-68. gillam, r.b. & johnston, j.r. (1992). spoken and written language relationships in language/learning-impaired and normally achieving school-age children. american speechlanguage-hearing association, 35, 1303-1315. haines, w.a. (1988). 'n ortodidaktiese evaluering van die effektiwiteit van die hulpklaspraktyk. m.ed(psig)-verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria. hallahan, d.p & kauffman, j.m. (1991). exceptional children introduction to special education (5th ed). new jersey: prentice-hall, inc. ! haylock, d. (1992). meeting the special needs of mathematical low attainers in the primaryl school. in jones, k. & charlton, t. (eds): learning difficulties in primary classrooms delivering the whole curriculum. london: routledge. hill, s.s. & haynes, w.o. (1992). language performance in lowachieving elementary schools students. language, speech and hearing services in schools,\23, 169-175. horowitz, r. (1990). discourse organization in oral and written language: critical contrasts for literacy and schooling. in de jong, j.h.a.l. & stevenson, d.k. (eds): individualizing the assessment of language abilities. clevedon: multilingual matters ltd. klein, m.d. & harris, k.c. (1986). classroom communication functions of four learning-handicapped students. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 17(4), 318-328. kriegler, s.m., du toit, e. & smart, p. (1990). ortodidaktiese pedagogiek: teorie en praktyk. universiteit van pretoria. leedy, p.d. (1993). practical research: planning and design (5th ed). new york: macmillan publishing company. lerner, j.w. (1993). learning disabilities theories, diagnosis & teaching strategies (6th ed). boston: houghton mifflin company. maxwell, s.e. & wallach, g.p. (1984). the language disabilities connection: symptoms of early language disability change over time. in wallach, g.p. & butler, k.g. (eds): language learning disabilities in school age children. london: williams & wilkins. moran, m.r. (1988). reading and writing disorders in the learning disabled student. in lass, n.j., mcreynolds, l.v., northern, j.l. & yoder, d.e. (eds): handbook of speech-language pathology and audiology. toronto: bc decker inc. naidoo, v., burden, α., topham, p. & singh, m. (1996) inclusion: a policy for meeting the needs of learners with special education needs. discussion document: gauteng department of education and culture. norris, j. & hoffman, p. (1993). whole language intervention for school-age children. san diego: singular publishing group, inc. oberstein, r. (1990). editorial. communiphon, 295, 1-2. oosthuizen, j.i. (1994). die verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van leergestremdes. m.log.-verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria. owens, r.e. (1992). language development an introduction (3rd ed.). new york: macmillan publishing company. sanger, d., hux, k. & griess, k. (1995). educators'opinions about speech-language pathology services in schools. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 26, 75-84. scarborough, h.s. & dobrich, w. (1990). development of children with early language delay. journal of speech and hearing research, 33, 70-83. schuele, m. & van kleeck, a. (1987). precursors to literacy: assessment and intervention. topics in language disorders, 7, 32-44. schumaker, j. & deshler, d. (1984). setting demand variables. topics in language disorders, 4(2), 22-40. smith-burke, m.t., deegan, d. & jaggar, a.m. (1991). whole language:. a viable alternative forspecial and remedial education? topics in language disorders, 11(3), 58-68. stackhouse, j. (1992). promoting reading and spelling skills through speech therapy. in fletcher, p. & hall, d. (eds): specific speech and language disorders in children: correlates, characteristics and outcomes. london: whurr publishers ltd. tempelhoff, e. (1994). hulpklasse baat die intelligent» kinders. die beeld. 21 april, ρ 8. uys, i.c. (1993). kommunikasiepatologie: onderrig vir die toekoms. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 40, 3-9. vorster, j. (1980a). handleiding vir die toets vir mondelinge taalproduksie. pretoria: suid-afrikaanse raad vir geesteswetenskaplike navorsing. vorster, j. (1980b). manual for the list for oral language production. pretoria: south african human sciences research council. wallach, g.p. (1990). magic buries celtics: looking for broader interpretations of language learning and literacy. topics in language disorders, 10(2), 63-80. welman, m.c.j. (1986). die doeltreffenheid van verbale ekspressie by taalleergestremde kinders. m.log.-verhandeliflg. universiteit van pretoria. williams, e. (1995). policy framework for the provision of professional services. support services workshop: johannesburg college of education. wright, j.a. (1992). collaboration between speech and language therapists and teachers. in fletcher, p. & hall, d. (eds): specific speech and language disorders in children: correlates, characteristics and outcomes. london: whurr publishers ltd. die suid-afrikaanse ydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 43 excess cerumen : failure rate of black and indian preschool children from durban on the middle ear screening protocol (mesp) daksha bhoola and rene hugo department of communication pathology university of pretoria abstract the aim of the study was to examine the failure rate of black and indian subjects within the excessive cerumen category of the middle ear screening protocol (mesp). the subjects included four to five year old black and indian children attending pre-schools in the durban central region. thus, a sample of 728 subjects (average age = 4.6 years), 312 black (135 male and 180 female) and 413 indian (223 male and 190 female) was screened using referral criteria based on a middle ear screening protocol (mesp). the results of the study indicated that a significant percentage of black (38,4%) and indian (49,9%) subjects failed, due to excessive cerumen. these results are discussed with reference to the literature. a cerumen management program has been outlined and recommended to be implemented in preschools in south africa. opsomming die doel van die studie was om ondersoek in te stel na die invloed van oormatige was op die faalkategoriee van 'n middeloorsiftingsprotokol. die proefpersone het bestaan uit vieren vyfjarige swarten indierkinders verbonde aan preprimere skole in die durban sentraalstreek. 'n totalegroep van 728proefpersone (gemiddelde ouderdom = 4.6jaar), waarvan 312 swart (135 manlik en 180 vroulik) en 413 indier (223 manlik en 190 vroulik) kleuters was, het 'n middeloorsiftingsprotokol ondergaan. die resultate van die studie dui daarop dat 'n betekenisvolle groot groep van die proefpersone -38.4% swart en 49.9% indier die siftinggefaal het as gevolg van oormatige was. hierdie resultate is bespreek aan die hand van relevante literatuur en opgevolg deur 'n voorgestelde was-hanteringsprogram wat moontlik sinvol in suid-afrikaanse kleuterskole geimplementeer kan word. key words: black and indian children, 4-5 years, cerumen management program, excessive cerumen, middle ear screening protocol. introduction the sancd (1990) classifies auditory dysfunction that occurs when the site is the external ear or middle ear, as conductive hearing loss. one of the commonest causes of conductive hearing impairment is the accumulation of cerumen in the external auditory meatus (eam) (northern & downs, 1984; martin, 1981; newby, 1979). total occlusion of the eam due to excessive or impacted cerumen is said to cause a threshold shift of about 45 70 db (chandler, 1964). in fact, bricco (1985) pointed out that impacted cerumen is regarded as an ear disease it can cause otitis externa, hearing loss, pain, itching and tinnitus (bullachanda & pears, 1992; roeser & crandell, 1991). furthermore, bricco (1985) stated that if cerumen impinges on the eardrum, a chronic cough may be triggered and persist until the cerumen is removed. myers and fueschel (1987) even related impacted cerumen to major psychiatric changes. bricco's (1985:241) investigation suggested "an association between the presence of impacted ear wax (cerumen) and subsequent middle ear or hearing problems, but a causal relationship is not inferred from the study". however, garber (1986) found that in some 30% of children, accurate diagnosis of acute otitis media (aom) requires cerumen removal. he stated that "you cannot assume that the heat of the middle ear infection will melt earwax : ceruminosis and acute otitis media can coexist". (garber 1986:151). the reasons for cerumen impaction are many. it may be due to increased secretory function of ceruminous glands, leading to more than normal production (mandour, el-ghazzawi, toppozoda & malaty, 1974) or failure of keratinocyte separation that occurs normally in the earcanal (robinson & hawke, 1990). anatomical abnormalities of the ear-canal, the improper use of cotton swabs, hearing aids, or a collapsed ear-canal have also been reported to obstruct extrusion of cerumen from the canal (ballachanda & peers, 1992). furthermore, the aggressive cleaning of soft wax by well-meaning parents can result in the wax becoming impacted against the tympanic membrane (bricco, 1985; newby, 1979; martin, 1981). in addition, ballachanda and peers (1992) stated that cerumen enlarges when it becomes soaked with water (e.g., after bathing, swimming) and can completely occlude the eam. thus, even the presence of excessive amounts of soft cerumen, poses a significant audiological problem since it can result in a mild conductive loss. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 it has been proven in studies on recurrent otitis media that mild and fluctuating conductive losses have negative effects on language and auditory function and on later educational achievements (gravel & wallace, 1992; boothroyd, 1982; northern & downs, 1984). this is equally true of excess cerumen and the negative consequences can be prevented through routine otoscopic examination of preschool children. in fact, the american food and drug administration (1977), cited by alpiner and mccarthy (1987), as well as asha (1990), identified excessive accumulation of cerumen as a condition that needed medical intervention. furthermore, american physicians are said to perform cerumen extraction on approximately 44 000 ears per year (sharp, wilson, ross & barr-hamilton 1990). according to burgess (1977), wax removal is done more than 40 000 times a week in the united kingdom. thus, otoscopic examination can "improve the timeliness of medical management for those in most urgent need." (roush 1990:367). the research design of the present study used a modified version of the revised asha (1990) protocol, referred to as the middle ear screening protocol (mesp). disorders of the outer and middle ear contribute to the conductive component of a hearing loss, which in many cases can be remediated with medical attention. it is possible that disease in the outer ear may spread to the middle ear and vice-versa (martin, 1981). therefore it is logical to include examination of the outer ear when considering middle ear disorders. it is for the above reasons, that the protocol used in this study, although being a middle ear screening protocol, is included in the examination of the outer ear. the mesp consists of three components; history, visual inspection and tympanometry. (refer to appendix a, table 1.) excess cerumen was identified using this protocol. martin (1991) stated that the problem of ear wax is a complex one and that no one wants to take the responsibility for the managing of it. in this regard two important questions needed to be answered: (i) "why haven't audiologists assumed the responsibility for cerumen management?" according to roeser and crandell (1991:52) a likely reason is that audiology's roots are in nonmedical institutions of higher education, where the philosophy is to avoid procedures that might be interpreted as "medical". moreover, in the early years of the profession, routine audiological protocols did not require placing objects in the earcanal. in fact, otoscopy was not performed or encouraged routinely. however, standard audiology protocols now require placing objects in the ear-canal and audiologists are familiar with the proper protocols to follow, as well as the possible contra-indications. the ear-canal is irrigated similarly to that of cerumen removal for caloric testing during electronystagmography. in addition, otoscopy is a mandatory prerequisite for proper audiological screening, evaluation and management. visual inspection of the ear-canal (otoscopy) is required in the asha screening guidelines (asha, 1989); moreover, the standard procedures in audiology manual developed by the veterans health services and research administration (vhsra, 1990) includes visual inspection of the ear as an audiological procedure. it is apparent that the above reasons are valid and that cerumen management should be the responsibility of the audiologist daksha bhoola & rene hugo in view of the new standard audiology protocols and the high frequency of occurrence in the population under study. (ii) should cerumen management be a part of audiology practice? asha (1990) defined the scope of practice in audiology to include : facilitating and conservation of auditory system function; preventing auditory system dysfunction; and selecting, fitting and dispensing amplification. in addition, asha (1996) included the following in the audiologist's scope of practice "otoscopic examination and external ear canal management for removal of cerumen in order to evaluate hearing or balance, make ear impressions, fit hearing protection or prosthetic devices, and monitor the continuous use of hearing aids". (asha, 1996:14). furthermore, the american academy of audiology (aaa) (1989:1), adopted a scope of practice statement that defines an audiologist as "a person who, by virtue of academic and clinical training and appropriate certification and/or licensure, is uniquely qualified to provide a comprehensive array of professional services related to the prevention, assessment and rehabilitation of auditory and vestibular impairments". in addition, "the audiologist is an independent practitioner, and may practice in a hospital, clinic, school, private practice or any other setting in which audiological services are relevant". in view of the limitations in providing comprehensive audiological services when excessive/impacted cerumen is present, both asha and aaa scope of practice statements give strong support and rationale for audiologists to engage in cerumen management. furthermore, considering the results of the present study on excessive cerumen in both race groups, it is time to recognize that audiologists can and should manage cerumen. it is evident from the results of the present study and the literature review presented above on the problem of excessive cerumen, that this is a serious problem in many countries, including south africa, and which needs urgent attention. the writer, therefore recommends a cerumen management programme which could be implemented in pre-schools and primary schools in south africa. 1 method research design i in order to realise the aim of the study, the methodology of research design used was the analytical (quantitative) survey method (leedy, 1989). aim the aim of the study was to examine the failure of black and indian preschool children within each category of the middle ear screening protocol (mesp). subjects a total of 728 randomly selected subjects (mean age 4,6 years), comprising 315 blacks (135 males, 180 females) and 413 indians (223 males, 190 females) contributed relevant data for the purpose of this study. available literature has indicated a paucity of information of middle ear the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) excess cerumen: failure rate of black and indian preschool children from durban on the middle ear screening protocol (mesp) disorders within the indian and black ethnic groups in south africa, which motivated the criterion of race. all subjects attended preschools located in the magisterial district of durban central. according to bess and humes (1990), children who reside in the inner city and attend day care centres are prone to suffer from outer and middle ear disorders. table 1 presents the subject characteristics of this study. all subjects were required to fall within the age range of 48-60 months inclusive (4-5 years) as a literature review has shown an increased prevalence of middle ear infection in this age range worldwide (davidson et al., 1988). 45 table 1 : subject characteristics n% mean age in years mean age in years black males females 315 (43) 135 (43 180 (57) 4,7 4,6 4.0-4,9 4.1-4,9 indian males females 413 (57) 223 (54) 190 (46) 4,6 4,6 4,2-5,0 4,0-4,8 total 728 data collection procedure screening. * immediate medical referral was made in cases of abnormally large canal volume (>1.0ml) estimates accompanied by low static admittance (<0.2ml) (when there was a reason to suspect a perforation of the tympanic membrane) (asha, 1990). * when tympanometric results were abnormal, (low static admittance (<0.2ml) and abnormal tympanometric peak pressure (< -200dapa)) rescreening was scheduled in 4-6 weeks from the date of the first screening. if the results were again abnormal, a medical referral was made. * normative data (table 2 of appendix a) based on the work of margolis and heller (1987) and asha (1978) were used for analysis of results in the procedures above. the pass-fail criteria are indicated in appendix a. * when a subject failed the first or second screening, the parents and school officials were informed by letter/report, of the test results and informed of a need for further evaluation. when medical review was indicated, parents were advised to consult with a general practitioner or their family doctor. a medical follow-up was requested from the attending doctor specifying his/her findings and treatment procedures for school records. data analysis procedures failure rate was computed in terms of percentages and numbers, and reflected in tables and graphs. the specific criteria for evaluation were as follows: all procedures for middle ear screening were completed on the same day, i.e., history, visual inspection and tympanometry for each subject. the screening was conducted on the school premises during school hours (08h0012h30). screening was scheduled for fall, i.e., between february and april 1992, to control for seasonal variations, since otitis media has its highest prevalence during the winter months (roust, 1990; sorensen, 1981). all subjects who met the subject1 selection criteria were included. the criteria included: race (black and indian), age (4-5 years old), and area of school (durban central region). * the teacher provided the biographical details as well as information pertaining to observable history of pain and ear drainage, wliich was recorded on the record form (appendix b). ; * following the collection of the above information, a visual inspection of the ear, head and neck was performed, using a welch-allen battery operated otoscope. otoscopic examination of each ear was conducted to identify, earcanal abnormalities, blood effusion, occlusion, inflammation, excessive cerumen, tumour or foreign material. each ear was then examined for eardrum abnormalities, and specifically for obvious inflammations, and severe retractions. * after visual inspection, typanometry was performed except when the earcanal was occluded with cerumen, or any other foreign material which prevented visual inspection of the tympanic membrane. according to asha (1990), excess cerumen is classified as soft or hard cerumen that totally occludes the ear canal and impacted wax. tympanometric measures were obtained using the grason-stadler 28a (grason-stadler, 1990), which was calibrated in january 1992, i.e., prior to (i) middle ear screening protocol (mesp) included history, visual inspection and tympanometry. the mesp consisted of two elements: outer ear tests and middle ear tests. (ii) failure on outer ear tests included: structural defects of the ear, head and neck ear-canal abnormalities, (i.e., bl to b9, refer to appendix b). (iii) failure on middle ear tests included: eardrum abnormalities tympanometry (iv) failure of a subject on any one of the categories, under outer ear and middle ear tests independently, was regarded as a fail. (v) failure of a subject on either ear on each category independently was regarded as a fail. (vi) both ears of a subject had to pass the three screening procedures in order for that subject to be regarded as a pass. the data were analysed in terms of failure rate of black and indian subjects within each category of the mesp. results were calculated using one subject as the statistical unit and classifying each subject according to the poorest result obtained in either ear. results the failure rate of black and indian subjects was computed within each category of the middle ear screening protocol. results in table 2 and figure 1 reflect the following failure rates for black, and indian subjects, within each category of the middle'ear screening protocol (mesp). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 daksha bhoola & rene hugo (i) history on history 0,3% (1) black and 0,5% (2) indian failed on otalgia, whereas 0,3% (1) black and 0,2% (1) indian failed on otorrhea. (ii) visual inspection visual inspection is divided into three subcategories, i.e., structural defects, earcanal abnormalities (eca) and eardrum abnormalities (eda). the results are as follows: * structural defects only one black (0,3%) failed on structural defects of the head. there were no failures in the indian group. * earcanal abnormalities (eca) there were no black or indian failures on occlusion bg categories. in the blood effusion category, 0,3% (1) blacks failed whereas no indians failed (b4). four point eight percent (15) blacks 2,2% (9) indians failed to inflammation (b6). of the 103 blacks, 32,7% and of the 188 indians, 45,5% failed due to excessive cerumen (b7) and 1,0% (3) blacks and 2,2% (9) indians failed to foreign material (b9). it is evident that a large percentage of indians (45,5%) and blacks (32,7%) failed due to excessive cerumen as compared to failure on any other category of earcanal abnormalities. furthermore, more indians (45,5) failed than blacks (32,7%) on excessive cerumen only. '' eardrum abnormalities (eda) there were no black or indian subjects failing due to severe retractions(b12). however, 1,0% (3) blacks and 0,5% (2) indians failed due to obvious perforations (b ) and 1,6% (5) blacks and 1,0% (4) indians failed due to obvious inflammation (b ). (iii) tympanometry on tympanometry measures, 1,0% (3) blacks and 0,5% (2) indians failed due to flat tympanogram (c ) and equivalent earcanal volume outside normal range (c ) ie. > 1,0cm3; 5,4% (17) blacks and 4,8% (20) indians failed due to low static admittance (c3), ie. <0,2cm3 and 8,3% (26) blacks and 10,4% (43) indians failed tympanometric peak pressure (c4) ie. <-200 dapa. failure rat· (%) 1 1 xj <5 118 bv j||\· h i ? j p a i a 2 b i b 2 s ! d * b s 5 e b π b l a c k ββ b1 β» btl β12 chcz ci c4 ^ indian figure 1 : failure rate (percentage) of black and indian subjects within each category of the mesp table 2 : failure rate (percentage and numbers) of black and indian subjects within each category of the middle ear screening protocol (mesp) middle ear screening protocol (mesp) outer ear tests middle ear tests subject history visual inspection subject structural defects ear-canal abnormalities ear-drum abnormalities tympanometry i a 2 b, b2 b3 b4 b5 b6 b7 b8 b9 b10 b u b12 c / c 2 c 3 et black 0,3 (1) 0,3 (1) 0 0,3 (1) 0 0.3 (1) 0 4.8 (15) 32.7 (0.3) 0 1.0 (3) 1.0 (3) 1.6 (5) 0 1.0 (3) 5.4 (17) 8.3 1 indian 0,5 (2) 0,2 (1) 0 0 0 0 0 2.2 (9) 45.5 (188) 0 2.2 (9) 0.5 (2) 1.0 (4) 0 0.5 (2) 4.8 (20) 10.4 (13) note: figures in parenthesis denote number of subjects failed. key for table 2 and figure 1 aj otalgia β j structural defect of the ear b3 structural defect of the neck b5 occlusion b7 excessive cerumen b9 foreign material b u obvious inflammation cj flat tympanogram (type b) cg static admittance (<0.2cm3) v b 2 " b 4 bsb„ β c 2 ottorrhea structural defect of the head blood effusion inflammation tumor . obvious perforation severe retraction equivalent ear-canal volume (> 1.0 cm3) tympanometric peak pressure (<-200 dapa) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) excess cerumen: failure rate of black and indian f middle ear screening protocol (mesp) discussion with reference to table 2 and figure 1, it is evident that a large percentage of both indians (45,5%) and blacks (32,7%) failed due to excessive cerumen as compared to failure on any other category of earcanal abnormalities. furthermore, more indians (45,5%) than blacks (32,7%) failed on excessive cerumen. ruben and fishman (1981) also found that the problem of impacted cerumen was common in their study of american infants. six of the 21 hearing impaired infants examined, presented with cerumen, this being a significant finding (rubin & fishman, 1981). bricco (1985), in her study of children younger than 7 years of age, found that 10 percent (35) of the subjects failed the screening because of impacted cerumen. however, 4,63% of the school children screened by audiologists watkins, moore and phillips (1984) failed their screening because of excessive cerumen in the eam. roeser and crandell (1991) emphasised that the incidence of excessive or impacted cerumen in children aged 6-17 years, is approximately 10%. in a pilot study conducted on indian children, (house of delegates, 1990) 87,8% of the subjects failed the hearing and immittance screening protocol. they concluded that "the majority of pupils failed one or all three subtests due to the presence of impacted wax unilaterally or bilaterally (house of delegates, 1990:3)". comparisons between the results of this study and the studies cited above must be drawn with caution due to the differences in age range of subjects in each study. however, in a study conducted by mandour, el-ghazzawi, toppozada and malaty (1974) on histological and histochemical study of the activity of ceruminous glands in normal and excessive wax accumulation in subjects aged one to thirty years, they showed no difference in the normal control groups either in the two sexes or at different ages. there appears to be no available information on the relationship between race and excessive accumulation of cerumen. comparatively, results on all other categories of the mesp was found to be nojt significant for both black and indian subjects. the striking results on the excessive cerumen category therefore indicate an urgent need for cerumen management. conclusion and recommendations the study revealed a high prevalence of excessive cerumen in both race groups. several studies in the literature have noted a high prevalence. as discussed in the introduction, excessive cerumen can cause a mild to moderate (fluctuating) conductive hearing loss. furthermore, medical consequences of impacted cerumen include tinnitus, pain, fullness in the ear and loss of hearing. the implication of this result is that there is an urgent need for cerumen management programs to be implemented and established with preschool children. the desired result of any screening program is the proper referral and care of individuals with identified problems. furthermore, universities can actively promote training and participation by students in cerumen extraction processes as part of their course requirements. unless audiologists manage cerumen they will be unable to provide comprehensive services to as many as 25-30% of certain populations (asha, 1990). a strong case is put forward for the audiologist's responsibility in cerumen manageihool children from durban on the 47 ment. further research in this respect is suggested. it is further suggested that teacher-parent education and general public awareness on the harmful effects of excessive cerumen, prevention and treatment of cerumen through organized programs need to be developed. although audiologists currently receive some theoretical training and clinical experience that qualifies them to manage cerumen, specific training in cerumen removal should be conducted before they engage in this activity (roeser & crandell, 1991). according to asha(1991), each audiologist who intends to perform cerumen removal procedures must ensure that he/she has acquired the knowledge and skills necessary to do each task necessary to the procedure. asha (1991) has outlined the tasks to be performed, the necessary proficiencies and the knowledge and skills necessary to do each task are summarised in appendix c. asha (1991) also stressed that the training should take place through direct supervision by a qualified professional in a setting allowing the trainee adequate clinical experience. the writer is in agreement with asha (1991) and recommends that asha's suggestions be followed in the south african pre-schools. however, each practitioner should consider the following precautions or circumstances prior to undertaking these procedures: (a) obtain a ruling from the appropriate professional board(s) to determine whether there are any limitations on the scope of audiology practice which restrict the performance of these procedures. (b) check professional liability insurance to ensure that there is no exclusion applicable to cerumen management. (c) check medical policy, institution insurance coverage and delineation of practice privileges for the specific institution to ensure that there are no restrictions applicable to an audiologist performing these procedures. (d) know whom to contact if emergency medical assistance is needed. (e) obtain informed consent from parents/guardian/ caregiver to proceed with cerumen management and maintain complete and adequate documentation. the program that follows, is extracted and modified from the dallas (texas) independent school district program (disd) (roeser, adam & watkins, 1991). the program has been modified to suit the preschools in the south african context. program procedures figure 2 presents a flow chart outlining the program. otoscopy should be performed by an audiologist prior to audiometric screening. audiometric screening should only be performed if both earcanals are unoccluded. if either or both canals are occluded, written permission from the parents/guardian is obtained for earcanal irrigation. if the child has no previous otologic history and permission is obtained, the audiologist must then instill wax softening agents into the earcanal twice daily for four to five days and the ears must be checked otoscopically. if cerumen is removed by the wax softening agent, audiometric and immittances screenings should be done. ears that remain occluded, must be irrigated until clean by the audiologist with a water pik using lukewarm wadie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 daksha bhoola & rene hugo yes 1 proceed withaudiometry no figure 2: flow chart of procedures for cerumen management preschool program reference: roeser, adam & watkins (1991: 47) ter (35 -39°c, 96 -100°f). care must be taken to avoid extreme temperatures which may cause nausea and vomiting. a low pressure setting, no higher than 2 on a scale of 0-10 on the water pik, should be used to dislodge the cerumen. audiometric and immittance screening should be performed once the ears are clear. these procedures are in accordance with roeser, adam and watkins, (1991). the audiologist must consider the contraindication of irrigation when implementing the programme. garber (1986) suggested that irrigation is contraindicated: (a) in young children or infants, because perforations are more likely in this age group; (b) in patients with tympanostomy tubes; (c) when tympanic membrane is perforated; (d) when surgery of middle ear has been recent. the program offers several significant advantages: (a) each child with impacted cerumen receives treatment necessary to ameliorate the problem. at present, patients with impacted cerumen are referred to ent specialists or doctors. often, these referrals are not completed and audiometric status remains unchanged, which may affect academic, psychoeducational and psychosocial development (roeser et al., 1991). '(b) the program will represent a time and monetary saving for parents because completing a referral often means half a day out of class for the child, medical expenses, the loss of parents'working time and salary. (c) reduced case load for ent specialists and doctors. (d)cerumen management will increase accessibility of the public to audiological practices and widen the use of our services. the procedure does have some inherent risks. injury and infection to the external auditory canal, perforation of the tympanic membrane, exacerbation of chronic mid die ear disease and damage to the ossicular chain can result (brooks, 1980). however, if procedures are performed by trained staff and applied judiciously, these risks are minimal to nonexistent (roeser et al., 1991). the program represents a modest investment of financial resources by the school system and time of the health care staff. roeser et al. (1991) reported data on more than 2000 ears that were irrigated without complications. the benefits to patient care and increased professional independence far outweigh the potential risks (roeser & crandell, 1991). it must be stressed that the proposed program is only a guideline and not a protocol that needs strict adherence. the validity and reliability of the program need to be determined. acknowledgement this article is based on master in logopaedics by the first author and she wishes to thank professor rene hugo and cyril govender for their support and assistance in the supervision of this study. references alpiner, j.g. & mccarthy, p.a. (1987). rehabilitative audiology : children and adults. baltimore : williams & wilkins. american academy of audiology. (1989). scope of practice working draft. american food and drug administration. (1977). in alpiner, j.g. & mccarthy, p.a. (1987). rehabilitative audiology: children and adults. baltimore : williams and wilkins co. american speech-language-hearing association. (1978). guidelines for acoustic immittance screening of middle ear function. american speech-language-hearing association, 21:550-558 american speech-language-hearing association. (1989). guidelines for screening hearing impairment and middle ear in disorders (draft: for peer review). 31:71-77. american speech-language-hearing association. (1990). guidelines for screening for hearing impairment and middle ear disorders). american speech-language-hearing association, 32:17-24. ' american speech-language-hearing association. (1991). external auditory canal examination and cerumen management. american speech-language-hearing association, 35 : 65-66. american speech-hearing association. (1996, spring). scope of practice in audiology. asha, 38:12-15. ; american speech-language-hearing association. (1996, spring). scope of practice in audiology. asha, 38:12-15. ballachanda, b.b. & peers, c.j.. (1992). cerumen management: instruments and procedures. american speech-languagehearing association, 43-46. bess, f.h. & humes, l.e. (1990) audiology : the fundamentals. baltimore : williams & wilkins. boothroyd, a. (1982). hearing impairments in young children. englewood cliffs, new york : prentice hall. bricco, e. (1985). impacted cerumen as a reason for failure in hearing conservation programs. journal of school health. 55 (6) : 240-241. brooks, d.n. (1980). acoustic impedance testing for screening auditory function in school children. part 1 and 2. in maico audiological library series, 15:2-5. burgess, e.h. (1977). earwax and the right way to use an ear syringe. nursing times, 73:1564-1565. chandler, r.j. (1964). partial occlusion of the external auditory meatus: its effect upon air and bone conduction hearing acuity. laryngoscope, 74:22-36. davidson, j., hyde, m.l. and alberti, p.w. (1988). epidemiology wax softening agents (4-5 days) i repeat otoscopy i oil dissolved cerumen i irrigate earisl repeat otoscopy audiometry aud immitance the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) r . r u m e n · f a i l u r e r a t e of b l a c k a n d i n d i a n p r e s c h o o l c h i l d r e n from d u r b a n o n t h e ^fiddle e a r s c r e e n i n g p r o t o c o l (mesp) 49 f hearing impairment in children. scandinavian audiology, ι 10*13-20 s l u p p r v (1986) removing impacted cerumen : procedures g a f o r vour practice'. patient care, 20, pp. 151-153. grason-stadler. (1990a). tympanometry in just seconds. grason qtnriler inc : littleton. ^ η stadler (1990b). gsi28a special auto tymp : instruction manual grayson stadler inc : littleton rravel j s & wallace, i.f. (1992). listening and language at 4 years of age : effects of early otitis media. journal of speech and hearing research, 35:588-595. house of delegates, (1990). study on hearing and immittance screening of class 2 pupils at two primary schools. unpublished report, department of health service and welfare. l edy pd (1989). practical research :planning and design. (4tn 6 p t i n e w york : macmillan publishers. mandour, m.a., el-ghazzawi, f„ toppozada, h.h. & malaty, h.a_ (1974) histological and histochemical study of the activity of ceruminous glands in normal and excessive wax accumulation. the journal of laryngology and otology, 88:1075-1085. mareolis r.h. & heller, j.w. (1987). screening tympanometry : criteria for medical referral. audiology, 26:197-208 martin, f.n. (1981). medical audiology : disorders of hearing. new jersey : prentice-hall, inc. martin, r.l. (1991). an effective program for managing ear wax. the hearing journal, 44:37. mvers β & fueschel, s.m. (1987). pseudodementia in the mentally retarded. clinical pediatrics, 26:275-263. newby, h.a. (1979). audiology (4th ed). new jersey : prentice northern^ j.l. & downs, m.p. (1984). hearing in children (3rd ed). london : williams & wilkins. robinson, a.c. & hawke, w.m. (1990). cerumen impaction: the unravelling of a mystery. otolaryngology head and neck surgery, 103, 2, 195. . roeser, j.r. & crandell, c. (1991). the a u d i o l o g i s t s responsibility in cerumen management. asha, 33 : 43-48. rouser r.j., adams, r.m. & watkins, s. (1991). cerumen management in hearing conversation : the dallas (texas independent school district program. journal of bchool health, 61(l):47-49. roush j (1990). identification of hearing loss and middle ear disease in preschool and school-age children. seminars m hearing, 11(4):357-371. ruben r j. & fishman, g. (1981). otological care of the hearing impaired child. in mencher, g.t. and gerbe, s.e. (eds). early management of hearing loss. london : grune and stratton. sharp j f, wilson, j.a. & barr-hamilton, r.m. (1990). ear wax removal: a survey of current practice. british medical journal, 301:1215-1253. sorensen h. (1981). the post-winter prevalence of secretory otitis media in four-year old children, judged by tympano-metry. in penha r and pizarro, p.n. (ed). proceedings of the fourth international symposium on acoustic impedance measurement. universidade nove de lisboa : 63-67. south african national council for the deaf. (1990). prevention of hearing impairment : summary. genetics and disability prevention cc. . , . . . . veterans health services and research administration (1990). standard procedures in audiology. washington, d.c. : audiology and speech pathology service. watkins, s. moore, t.h. & phillips, j. (1984). clearing impacted ears. american journal of nursing, 84:1107.0 a p p e n d i x a t a b l e 1 rreferral criteria : m i d d l e e a r s c r e e n i n g p r o t o c o l ( m e s p ) i h i s t o r y (a) otalgia (b) o t o r r h e a ii v i s u a l i n s p e c t i o n of the e a r (a) structural dejfect of the ear, h e a d or n e c k (b) e a r c a n a l abnormalities b l o o d or effusion o c c l u s i o n i n f l a m m a t i o n e x c e s s i v e c e r u m e n , tumour, foreign material (c) e a r d r u m abnormalities o b v i o u s perforation o b v i o u s i n f l a m m a t i o n severe retraction iii t y m p a n o m e t r y (a) flat t y m p a n o g r a m and equivalent earcanal volume ( v c ) outside normal range, o c c u r r e n c e s in a 4 6 w e e k interval. modified from : a s h a (1990) a n d a s h a (1978) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 daksha bhoola & rene hug table 2 : interim norms (means and 90% ranges) for static admittance (peak y), equivalent earcanal volume (vcc) and tympanometric peak pressure (tpp) peaky cm3/ml* v ec cm'/ml* tpp dapa means 90% range means 90% range means 90% range children 0.05 0.2-0.9 0.7 0.4-1.0 100 -200 +100 the values were extracted from margolis & heller (1987) who employed an acoustic immittance screening instrument (226-hz) probe tone; pump speed -200 dapa/s that automatically compensated for earcanal volume by subtracting the admittance at 200 dapa from all values. normative values for children were obtained from preschool-aged children (3-5 years). *cm3 and ml are equivalent units (asha, 1990). modified : roush (1990) from asha (1978) appendix β pupil record form personal details reference no: name: sex: school d.o.b date of test: age: date of retest: race :, audiologist: daksha bhoola key: red = right ear; blue = left ear a. history first screen second screen a.l otolgia yes no yes no otorrhea yes no yes no b. visual inspection structural inspection of the b.l ear yes no yes no ' b.2 head yes no yes no i b.3 neck yes no yes no ' ear canal abnormalities b.4 blood effusion yes no yes no b.5 occlusion yes no yes no b.6 inflammation yes no yes no. b.7 excessive cerumen yes no yes no b.8 tumor yes no yes no b.9 foreign material yes no yes no eardrum abnormalities b.10 obvious perforation yes no yes no b . l l obvious inflammations yes no yes no b.12 severe retractions yes no yes no the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) excess cerumen: failure rate of black and indian preschool children from durban on the middle ear screening protocol (mesp) 51 first screen c. tympanometry c 1 tympanogram c 2 static admittance c.3 earcanal volume c.4 tympanometric peak pressure d. summary d.l history d.2 visual inspection d.3 tympanometry d.4 send letter to parent d.5 send letter to ent d.6 retest a <0,2 <0,4 <-200dapa as β 0,2-0,9 0,4-1,0 -200-+100dapa fail fail fail yes yes yes c ad >0.9 >1,0 +loodapa actual value r l pass pass pass no no no second screen c. tympanometry c . l tympanogram c.2 static admittance c.3 earcanal volume c.4 tympanometric peak pressure a <0,2 <0,4 200dapa as β 0,2-0,9 0,4-1,0 -200-+100dapa c ad >0.9 >1,0 +loodapa actual value r l d. summary d.l history d.2 visual inspection d 3 tympanometry d.4 send letter to parent d.5 send letter to ent d.6 retest fail fail fail yes yes yes pass pass pass no no no appendix c summary of tasks, i^roiiciency, iuiuwi task ! bugc auu ——1 — proficiency knowledge/skills needed inspect visually via hand-held otoscope or head-light (mirror)! and speculum the eac and tm for presence'of obstructing material, evidence of lesions or ongoing infectious process, evidence of anatomical anomalies that may affect the accuracy of immittance or caloric irrigations. visual inspection of the eac and tm. 1. knowledge of anatomy, physiology and pathophysiology of the eac and tm. 2. knowledge of common medical or post-surgical conditions of the eac, tm or middle ear that alter the appearance and/or function of the eac and/or tm. . 3. skill in the use of otoscopy. 4. skill in the interpretation of visual inspection of the eac and tm inspect the eac and tm visually via hand-held pneumatic otoscope or head light (mirror) and seigel scope for determining the mobility of the tm. determination of appropriateness of tm mobility. 1. knowledge of anatomy, physiology and pathophysiology of the eac and tm. 2. knowledge of common medical or postsurgical conditions of the eac, tm or middle ear that alter the appearance and/or function of the eac and/or tm. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 daksha bhoola & rene hug 3. skill in the use of otoscopy and pneumatic otoscopy. 4. skill in the interpretation of visual inspection of the eac and tm. inspect the eac and tm visually prior to and following caloric irrigation, immittance, ear mould impression, real ear acoustic measurements or noninvasive electrocochlegraphy for purpose of documenting status of the eac and tm after one of these procedures is performed. visual inspection of the eac and tm 1. knowledge of common medical or post surgical conditions of the eac, tm or middle ear that alter the appearance and/or function of the eac and/or tm. 2. skill in the use of otoscopy and pneumatic otoscopy. 3. skill in the interpretation of visual inspection of the eac and tm. determine if occluding material visualized in the eac is cerumen and if it can be removed comfortably and safely without the use of an operating microscope. recognizing cerumen versus other occluding versus other occluding materials, and determining its need for removal and the most effective method of removal 1. skill in the use of otoscopy and pneumatic otoscopy. 2. skill in the interpretation of visual inspection of the eac and tm. 3. skill in cerumen removal by a variety of techniques and equipment. determine if the procedure to be performed should be deferred, based on eac and tm inspection, and if referral to an otolaryngologist is indicated. determining the status of the eac and tm relative to the needs of the procedure to be performed. 1. knowledge of anatomy, physiology and pathophysiology of the eac and tm. 2. knowledge of common medical or postsurgical conditions of the eac or tm or middle ear that alter the appearance and/or function of the eac and/or tm. 3. skill in the use of otoscopy and pneumatic otoscopy. 4. skill in the interpretation of visual inspection of the eac and tm. 5. skill in cerumen removal by, e.g., use of cerumen loop and hand-held otoscopic device; use of cerumen loop, head mirror and hand-held speculum; use of material for softening and gentle water irrigation or a combination of these methods. establish appropriate protocol with medical personnel to handle eac abrasion or laceration that could result from cerumen removal. determining need for other medical service involvement in the care of the eac and tm. 1. knowledge of anatomy, physiology and pathophysiology of the eac and tm. 2. knowledge of common medical or t postsurgical conditions of the eac; tm or middle ear that alter the j appearance and/or function of the eac and/or tm. select appropriate method and remove occluding cerumen or refer to an otolaryngolist for removal once determining that: a. use of a microscope may be required; b. the cerumen is too close to the tm; c..the occluding material is not cerumen; d. the comfort or safety of the patient may be comprised determining if occluding material is cerumen and, if it is cerumen, determining if it can be removed safely and comfortably without the use of an oprating microscope. 1. knowledge of anatomy, physiology, and pathophysiology of the eac and tm. 2. skill in cerumen removal by a variety of techniques and equipment key : eac external auditory canal, tympanic membrane reference : asha (1991:66) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 41 die bydrae van basiese navorsing in kliniese toepassings met verwysing na kogleere inplantings tania hanekom en johan j. hanekom departement elektriese, elektroniese en rekenaar-ingenieurswese universiteit van pretoria opsomming hierdie artikel bespreek die waarde van basiese navorsing soos toegepas op kogleere inplantings. die artikel is gerig op klinici en oudioloe wat in die veld van kogleere inplantings werk, ofwat in hierdie veld belangstel. die artikel gee ook 'n meer algemene inleiding tot modellering vir navorsers in die kliniese omgewing. dit gee 'n wegsprihgpunt vir kogleere inplanting navorsing en gee 'n oorsig oor die toepassing van basiese navorsing in nuwe ontwikkeling in kogleere inplantings. daar word gewys wat tot dusver bereik is en watter probleme nog bestaan. die rol van multidissiplinere navorsingspanne by die oplossing van hierdie probleme word bespreek. eksperimentele navorsing en modellering werk saam om probleme op te los en nuwe ontdekkings te doen. die belangrikheid van modellering as 'n gereedskapstuk vir basiese navorsing word beklemtoon. abstract this article discusses the value of basic research as applied to cochlear implants. the article is aimed at clinicians and audiologists who are working in the field of cochlear implants or who are interested in this field. the article also gives a more general introduction to modelling for researchers in the clinical environment. it provides an entry point to cochlear implant research and reviews the application of basic research to new developments in cochlear implants. it is shown what has been achieved so far and which problems still exist. the role of multidisciplinary research teams to solve these problems is discussed. experimental research and modelling co-operate to solve problems and make new discoveries. the importance of modelling as a tool for basic research is emphasized. sleutelwoorde: kogleere inplantings, elektriese stimulasie, modellering, multidissiplinere navorsing, basiese navorsing. inleiding daar is dikwels die persepsie dat 'n kloof bestaan tussen die werelde van die navorser en die klinikus. die klinikus mag voel dat navorsing soms isoteriese probleme aanspreek wat van min praktiese waarde is. daarenteen mag die navorser voel dat die klinikus nie genoeg begrip het van die onderliggende beginsels waarop sy of haar werk berus nie. hierdie artikel spreek hierdie probleem aan uit die oogpunt van die navorser. meer spesifiek, in hierdie artikel word menslike gehoor en kogleere inplantings (clark, 1993) gebruik as voertuig om te demonstreer watter basiese navorsing in hierdie veld nodig is en wat die praktiese impak daarvan vir die oudioloog en klinikus is. die artikel is dus gerig op lede van die mediese span wie se betrokkenheidby gehoor en kogleere inplantings primer klinies van aard is (spesifiek oudioloe en klinici). die doel is om te wys wat die rol en toepaslikheid van basiese navorsing is en ook om 'n begrip te vestig van die probleme wat navorsers aanspreek en die tegnieke wat hulle gebruik. 'n omvattende bronnelys word ingesluit as inleiding tot die literatuur in die veld. , die benadering wat in die artikel gevolg word, is om 'n beskrywing te gee van die aard van navorsing, asook die gereedskap en metodes wat navorsers gebruik en om die bydrae van basiese navorsing in neuroprostetiese rehabilitasie met spesifieke verwysing na kogleere inplantings in perspektief te stel. die artikel beklemtoon spesifiek modellering as 'n belangrike gereedskapstuk in basiese navorsing. die aard van navorsing navorsing in neuroprostetiese rehabilitasie kan in drie kategoriee verdeel word: kliniese navorsing, tegnologiese navorsing (ingenieurswese) en basiese navorsing. kliniese navorsing is gemik op (1) die habilitasie en rehabilitasie van gestremde persone (spesifiek gehoorgestremde persone in hierdie artikel) deur die keuse van geskikte hulpmiddels uit dit wat beskikbaar is, (2) toepassing van die hulpmiddels (bv. die inplantering van prosteses), (3) navorsing omtrent die beste maniere om hierdie hulpmiddels te gebruik (insluitende verbeterde kliniese prosedures), en (4) die ontwikkeling van effektiewe nasorg'programme. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 tania hanekom & johan j. hanekom hierdie aspekte word hoofsaaklik bedryf deur oudioloe en klinici en val buite die bespreking van hierdie artikel. tegnologiese navorsing in neuroprostetiese rehabilitasie word hoofsaaklik bedryf deur ingenieurs, maar dit behoort duidelik te wees dat dit nie in isolasie kan gebeur nie. ingenieurs ontwikkel nuwe hulpmiddels, dikwels na aanleiding van behoeftes wat gestel word deur gestremde mense of deur medici. behoeftes mag bestaan-lank voor die tegnologie ver genoeg ontwikkel het om die spesifieke probleem te kan aanspreek. kogleere inplantings is een van die suksesverhale van biomediese ingenieurswese. graham clark het in die vorige eeu reeds besef dat die tegnologie ryp was vir 'n tegnologiese oplossing vir doofheid. hoewel hy beskou word as een van die pioniers van hierdie tegnologie, het hy gebou op vorige navorsing (andreef, gersuni & volokhov, 1934; brummer & turner, 1975; house & urban, 1973; simmons, mongeon, lewis & huntington, 1964). 'n verdere bespreking van die proses betrokke by die ontwikkeling van nuwe tegnologie val buite die bestek van hierdie artikel. basiese navorsing gee die grondslag waarop die praktiese oplossing van probleme gebou is, maar dit kan die uiteindelike tegnologiese toepassing met baie jare vooruitloop. volta (1800) het byvoorbeeld reeds twee eeue terug die eerste keer met elektriese stimulasie geeksperimenteer. toepassings, of potensiele toepassings, gee rigting aan basiese navorsing. die doel van basiese navorsing is eerstens om 'n fundamentele onderbou te verskaf waarop kliniese toepassings en tegnologiese ontwikkeling baseer kan word. twee voorbeelde word gegee. eerstens, sonder die begrip van die elektriese stimuleerbaarheid van senuweeweefsel (bv. galvani, 1791; hodgkin & huxley, 1952), sou dit nie moontlik wees om die tegnologie van kogle§re inplantings te ontwikkel nie. tweedens, die diagnostiese hulpmiddels wat in die oudiologie gebruik word, spruit voort uit die groot hoeveelheid navorsing in die psigoakoestiek wat oor die afgelope eeu gedoen is (richards, 1976). basiese navorsing stel dus ten doel eerstens om 'n beter begrip te verkry van die fundamentele werking van die onderliggende biologie en fisiologie van 'n lewende stelsel en tweedens om die reaksie van die stelsel op spesifieke insette of stimuli te voorspel. binne die konteks van neuroprosteses is die nut van basiese navorsing dus om (1) die wetenskaplike kennis omtrent die werking van die normale en elektries-gestimuleerde biologiese stelsel uit te brei (bv. clark, shute, shepherd & carter, 1997; shannon, 1993), (2) die funksionering van prosteses te verbeter deur nuwe kennis omtrent die werking van die biologiese stelsel toe te pas gedurende die ontwerp van nuwe generasies tegnologiese hulpmiddels (bv. cords, reuter, issing, sommer, kuzma & lenarz, 2000), en (3) om die effek van elektriese stimulasie te verklaar vir verskillende inplantings wat op die mark beskikbaar is bv. (tye-murray, lyier, woodworth & gantz, 1992). hieronder word aangedui wat die navorsingsvrae omtrent kogleere inplantings is en watter gereedskap gebruik word by navorsing. eerstens word die probleem van kogleere inplantings kortliks geskets. die kogleere inplantingsprobleem kogleere inplantings kan vanuit beide 'n kliniese oogpunt en bio-ingenieurswese oogpunt beskou word as suksesvol (national institutes of health, 1995). die inplantings is veilig en betroubaar. kliniese prosedures en prosesse vir voorsorg, insorg en nasorg is gevestig. inplantings is voordelig vir die gebruiker, vir wie dit in kontak plaas met die klankomgewing. inplantings word ook as finansieel effektief beskou. daar is egter vele onopgeloste probleme, insluitend onvoorspelbaarheid van resultate (clark, 1996), groot interpasientvariasie in sukses (kou, shipp & nedzelski, 1994), baie gebruikers se lae spraakverstaanbaarheid in 'n normale spraakomgewing (kou, shipp & nedzelski, 1994; clark, 1996), meeste gebruikers se musiekpersepsie is swak (fujita & ito, 1999), sommige gebruikers het steeds balansprobleme (kou, shipp & nedzelski, 1994), baie gebruikers kan nie telefonies kommunikeer nie en sukkel in ruiserige omstandighede (hirsch, 1993). die ideaal is dat 'n gebruiker se inplanting onsigbaar van buite is, lang lewensduur het, lang batterylewe het en klankkwaliteit gee wat naby aan normale gehoor is. kogleere inplantings se ontwerp is gebaseer op drie onderliggende beginsels: (1) 'n senuweevesel kan geaktiveer of gestimuleer word deur elektriese stroom deur die vesel te laat vloei, (2) senuweevesels in die koglea is tonotopies gerangskik (greenwood, 1990) en (3) frekwensie-inligting word gekodeer in beide tydpatrone sowel as die plek van aktiwiteit in die cochlea (evans, 1978). die tonotopiese rangskikking van die senuweevesels in die koglea verwys na die rangskikking van senuweevesels volgens toonhoogte oor die lengte van die koglea. stimulasie van senuweevesels naaste aan die basis van die koglea is verantwoordelik vir persepsie van hoe toonhoogtes terwyl senuweevesels nader aan die apeks laer toonhoogtes waarneem. deur dus stroom deur die senuweevesels te laat vloei op spesifieke plekke in die koglea, kan verskillende toonhoogtesensasies geskep word. vokoder-tipe strategiee maak gebruik van hierdie idee (bv. die speak strategie, loizou, 1999). daarenteen fokus strategiee soos cis (loizou, 1999) op die behoud van tydpatrone. in teenstelling met akoestiese opwekking van senuwees in 'n normale koglea, word groot getalle senuweevesels gelyktydig geaktiveer tydens elektriese stimulasie (javel, 1990). een rede hiervoor is dat daar ongeveer 30 000 senuwees in die koglea is (allen, 1985) wat met slegs 16 tot 22 elektrodes geaktiveer moet word (clark, 1993; kessler, 1999). verder kan die stimulasiestrategiee wat gebruik word om klanklinligting oor te dra nie die tydof ruimtelike patrone waarvolgens die gehoorsenuweevesels akoesties geaktiveer word perfek namaak nie (javel, 1990; miller, abbas, rubinstein, robinson, matsuoka & woodworth, 1998). die stroomverspreiding om die elektrodes tesame met die stimulasiestrategie het waarskynlik 'n groot effek op die klanksensasie wat 'n persoon met 'n kogleere inplanting waarneem (clark, 1996). die uitdaging is dus om die verspreiding van die stimulasiestroom by die senuwees te beheer en om die stimulasiestrategie te optimeer in 'n poging om die natuurlike senuweeaktiveringspatrone in die koglea na te maak (rubinstein, wilson, finley & abbas, 1999). twee aanverwante navorsingsdoelwitte wat hieruit spruit, is (1) om metodes te vind om die verspreiding van die stimulasiestroom by die senuwees te beheer (bv. cords, reuter, issing," sommer, kuzma & lenarz, 2000) en om (2) die proses waarop die sentrale gehoorstelsel die inligting wat tydens stimulasie oorgedra word integreer, verwerk en interpreteer verder te ontsyfer (bv. hanekom & kriiger, 2001; zeng & shannon, 1999) sodat die stimulasiestrategie daarvolgens ontwerp the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die bydrae van basiese navorsing in kliniese toepassings met verwysing na kogleere inplantings 43 kan word. dis byvoorbeeld nog nie duidelik of tydof plekkodering dominant is nie (moore & sek, 1996). die gereedskap wat gebruik word vir basiese navorsing basiese navorsing in kogleere inplantings sluit in neurofisiologiese laboratoriumeksperimente, psigoakoestiese eksperimente met normaalhorende persone sowel as gebruikers van inplantings, en modellering van die perifere en sentrale gehoorstelsel, beide wanneer dit akoesties gestimuleer word en wanneer dit elektries gestimuleer word. om die navorsingsdoelwitte genoem in die vorige paragraaf te kan behaal, bv. om die verspreiding van die stimulasiestroom te beheer en om die sentrale verwerkingsprosesse in die brein te verstaan, is dit belangrik om die fisiese en biologiese beginsels met standaardmetodes te kan naspeur en te kan beskryf. analitiese navorsingstegnieke en prosedures in die neurofisiologie en psigoakoestiek is goed gevestig in standaardprosedures oor 'n tydperk van baie dekades. beskrywingstegnieke kom uit die domein van ingenieurs, fisici en wiskundiges. modellering ingenieurs skep dikwels wiskundige modelle om prosesse voor te stel en die effek van veranderings aan 'n stelsel te voorspel. 'n model is 'n wiskundige beskrywing vaii 'n natuurlike verskynsel in hierdie geval dus 'n wiskundige beskrywing van die werking van die sentrale gehoorstelsel of 'n wiskundige beskrywing van die gei'nplanteerde koglea. die ingenieur of natuurwetenskaplike skep dus 'n stelsel van wiskundige vergelykings waarmee die gedrag van die normale gehoorstelsel of van die elektriesgestimuleerde gehoorstelsel beskryf kan word. die vergelykings kan met analitiese metodes (bv. rubinstein, soma & spelman, 1985; siebert, 1970) of met numeriese metodes (bv. finley, wilson & white, 1990; rattay & aberham, 1993) opgelos word. numeriese oplossings word verkry met ' rekenaarsimulasies. modelle kan verder varieer vanaf streng biologiesgebaseerde modelle tot swartkassiemodelle. die term swartkassiemodel word dikwels deur ingenieurs gebruik vir 'n stelsel waarvan mens die inset en die uitset kan meet, maar geen kennis het oor wat binne-in die stelsel (die "swart kassie") sit nie. voorbeelde word hieronder gegee. streng biologiesgebaseerde modelle daarenteen inkorporeer soveel moontlik bestaande kennis oor die biologiese anatomie en fisiologie. sulke modelle mag byvoorbeeld die funksie van 'n spesifieke stelsel in die brein modelleer deur. 'n groot aantal senuwees te modelleer en dan hierdie senuwees saam te voeg in 'n groter struktuur (bower, 1990). meer spesifiek, in sulke modelle mag elke senuwee wat modelleer word beskryf word deur 'n volledige hodgkin-huxleymodel (hodgkin & huxley, 1952). die hodgkin-huxleymodel is een van 'n. aantal biologiesgetroue senuweemodelle en gebruik 'n hele aantal wiskundige vergelykings om die werking van 'n enkele senuweesel te beskryf. swartkassiemodelle beskryf bloot inset-uitsetdata deur 'n wiskundige vergelyking sonder die inagneming van die biologie. sommige senuweemodelle modelleer die senuwee bloot as 'n generator van senuweepulse (bv. gabbiani & koch, 1996). in die eenvoudigste vorm pas swartkassiemodelle bloot 'n kurwe aan die data en aanvaar geen begrip van die werking van die stelsel wat dit modelleer nie. swartkassiemodelle kan baie beperk wees in terme van die datastelle wat voorspel kan word. tussen streng biologiese modelle en swartkassiemodelle bestaan 'n hele kontinuum van modelle wat tot 'n mindere of meerdere mate bestaande kennis oor die biologie inkorporeer. sommige modelle ignoreer doelbewus bestaande kennis (dus is meer gei'dealiseerd as streng biologiesgebaseerde modelle) met die doel om randfaktore buite rekening te laat (soos byvoorbeeld variasies tussen mense) om te kan deurdring tot die kernbewerkings wat die stelsel wat ondersoek word, uitvoer. modelle word gekarakteriseer deur die modelstruktuur en die parameters. hierdie terme word met 'n voorbeeld verduidelik. om 'n sekere funksie (bv. diskriminasie van twee suiwer tone) uit te voer is 'n netwerk van neurone op 'n sekere manier aan mekaar geskakel. vir sommige stelsels in die liggaam is hierdie struktuur in minder of meer detail bekend, bv. struktuur van die piriforme korteks, die primere sentrum vir die verwerking van olfaktoriese inligting (bower, 1990), en die struktuur van die sentrale ouditiewe stelsel, (brugge, 1992; ehret, 1997). wanneer die struktuur van 'n model gedefinieer word, moet daar onder andere besluit word hoeveel neurone die model moet bevat, wat die aard van die sinapse (inhiberend of eksiterend) moet wees, en wat die plasing van die sinapse in die struktuur moet wees. die modelstruktuur kan afgelei word uit die anatomie (bower, 1990). nadat die struktuur bekend is, mag sekere parameters steeds onbekend wees. die eienskappe van die senuweemembraan (bv. hoe sterk dit reageer op 'n stimulus), die spoed waarteen senuwees aksiepotensiale gelei en die dikte van die senuwees is voorbeelde van modelparameters. bower (1990) gee 'n gedetailleerde voorbeeld van so 'n rekenaarmodel vir die piriforme korteks. modellering is dikwels 'n iteratiewe proses. gegrond op die navorser se begrip van die prosessering wat uitgevoer word in die stelsel wat gemodelleer word, bepaal die navorser 'n modelstruktuur. parameters vir die model moet dan gemeet word, of kan soms uit die literatuur verkry word. dikwels is dit nodig om modelparameters te skat en soms is dit voldoende as die skatting net naastenby reg is. met struktuur en parameters bekend, kan rekenaarsimulasies dan met die model uitgevoer word. die gedrag van die verskynsel wat gemodelleer word (die uitsette van die model), word tipies beskryf in terme van meetbare hoeveelhede soos byvoorbeeld die vuurtempo op enkelsenuwees, ruimtelike aktiveringspatrone op 'n aantal senuwees, gehoordrempels en drempels van frekwensiediskriminasie. dit is nodig dat die uitsette van 'n model ooreenstem met meetbare hoeveelhede sodat die werking van die model met fisiese metings bevestig kan word. as die model se gedrag goed is, m.a.w. die model kan gemete data getrou namaak (dit kan absolute waardes en tendense voorspel), gee dit vertroue dat die keuse van die struktuur goed was. soms word tendense reg voorspel, maar die groottes van die gemete data en voorspelde data stem nie ooreen nie. dit kan wees omdat die struktuur van die model reg is, maar die parameters foutief geskat of gemeet is. aan die ander kant kan dit gebeur dat die model hoegenaamd glad nie die gemete data kan voorspel nie, en dan is die eerste vermoede dat die modelstruktuur verkeerd is. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 'η belangrike vraag wat oor elke model beantwoord moet word is: kan,die model gemete data buite die datastel waarvoor die model geskep is, verklaar? indien nie, moet die model verder ontwikkel word. hoe meer datastelle 'n model kan verklaar, hoe meer vertroue kry die navorser dat die verwerking van die onderliggende stelsel verstaan word en dat die model se struktuur dus reg is. as voorbeeld, as 'n model geskep is om frekwensiediskriminasie van enkeltone te voorspel, maar dit kan ook die diskriminasie van komplekse tone voorspel, gee dit heelwat vertroue in die geldigheid van die model. soos modelle verder ontwikkel word, groei begrip van die stelsel wat gemodelleer word. waarvoor word modelle gebruik? modelle van die gehoorstelsel kan nie die effek van stimulasie of die gedrag van die gehoorstelsel perfek voorspel nie, omdat hulle 'n vereenvoudiging van die werklikheid voorstel. dit is byvoorbeeld onmoontlik om al die interpersoonvariasies in die aantal residuele senuwees in die kogleas van dowe persone in ag te neem. wat is die nut van modelle dan? 'n volmaakte voorstelling van die werklikheid is nie noodwendig noodsaaklik, of beskikbaar, om die funksionering van 'n stelsel te beskryf nie. kogleere inplantings is 'n goeie voorbeeld hiervan, omdat verbeterings aan die inplantings reeds suksesvol aangebring is op grond van onder andere modelleringstudies, ten spyte daarvan dat die funksionering van die gehoorstelsel nie volledig verstaan word nie (clark, 1996). as 'n model dus die belangrikste eienskappe van 'n stelsel so korrek kan voorspel dat die inligting bruikbaar is, is die model nuttig. heelwat modelle is byvoorbeeld geskep om psigoakoestiese data te voorspel vanuit neurofisiologiese data. sulke modelle het onlangs gewys dat ruis 'n positiewe rol speel in gehoor (bruce, white, irlicht, o'leary & clark, 1999; zeng, fu & morse, 2000). op grond hiervan is nuwe algoritmes voorgestel wat meer ruis sal bewerkstellig in vuurpatrone as gevolg van elektriese stimulasie (bruce, irlicht, white, o'leary, dynes, javel & clark, 1999; bruce, white, irlicht, o'leary & clark, 1999; rubinstein, wilson, finley & abbas, 1999; white, rubinstein & kay, 2000). modelle van die gei'nplanteerde koglea is ook geskep om die stroomverspreiding om die elektrodes te voorspel (finley, wilson & white, 1990; frijns, de snoo & schoonhoven, 1995; girzon, 1987; t. hanekom, 2001; rattay, leao & felix, 2000). hierdie modelle het gewys dat senuwees meer selektief gestimuleer kan word as die elektrodes nader aan die senuwees geplaas word. op grond van modelleringsdata soos hierbo en metings in die laboratorium (shepherd, hatsushika & clark, 1993) is nuwe elektrodes, soos naby-modiolus elektrodes (donnelly, cohen, xu, xu & clark, 1995; treaba, xu, xu & clark, 1995), en 'n elektrodeposisioneerder (cords, reuter, issing, sommer, kuzma & lenarz, 2001), ontwikkel. nog 'n belangrike gebruik van betroubare modelle is dat dit die navorser in staat stel om konsepte te toets in rekenaarsimulasies, eerder as om direk eksperimente op mense of diere te doen. eksperimentele navorsing eksperimentele navorsing sluit 'n wye veld in en is gerig op die fisiese kwantifisering van parameters waarvoor daar ontwerp moet word in neuroprostetiese toestelle. ekspetania hanekom & johan j. hanekom rimentele navorsing is nader aan die verwysingsraamwerk van die klinikus of oudioloog en sal nie hier in diepte bespreek word nie. slegs enkele navorsingsgebiede word ter illustrasie genoem. ingenieursaspekte wat tipies tydens basiese navorsing aangespreek word sluit in bioaanpasbaarheid, veiligheid en betroubaarheid van die materiale wat ge'inplanteer word (brummer & turner, 1975; clark, shepherd, patrick, black & tong, 1983; donaldson, donaldson & brindley, 1985; hambrecht, 1985; house & urban, 1973; lenhart, 1992; seldon, dahm, clark & crowe, 1994; van noordt & black, 1981); betroubaarheid van die stelsel (soma, 1986); korrosie van die elektrodemateriale (donaldson & donaldson, 1986; donaldson, donaldson & brindley, 1985; hanekom & hanekom, 1998; johnson & hench, 1977; shepherd,/murray, houghton & clark, 1985); vervaardiging van elektrodes, gei'nplanteerde elektronika en eksterne elektronika (clark, shepherd, patrick, black & tong, 1983; hanekom, hanekom & marais, 1998; lauridsen, giinthersen, bonding & tos, 1982); en betroubaarheid van elektronika en hermetiese verseeling (donaldson, 1988; soma, 1990). neurofisiologiese eksperimente is gerig op die bepaling van die biologiese stelsel se eienskappe en werking waarbinne die prostese moet funksioneer. in kogleere prosteses is kennis oor die werking van senuweevesels en die seinverwerking deur die senuweevesels en sentrale gehoorstelsel van primere belang. fisioloe het meettegnieke ontwikkel en metings gedoen op senuwees om die meganisme waarvolgens senuwees geaktiveer word na te speur (frankenhaeuser, 1956; hodgkin & huxley, 1952; schwarz & eikhof, 1987), om eienskappe van senuwees te meet (verveen, 1962), om senuwees se reaksie op elektriese stimulasie te meet (miller, abbas, rubinstein, robinson, matsuoka & woodworth, 1998; rattay, 1999; rubinstein, wilson, finley & abbas, 1999; shepherd, hatsushika & clark, 1993; stypulkowski & van den honert, 1984; van den honert & stypulkowski, 1984; van den honert & stypulkowski, 1987a; van den honert & stypulkowski, 1987b) en om skade aan senuwees as gevolg van elektriese stimulasie te bepaal (huang & shepherd, 1999; mccreery, agnew, yuen & bullara, 1992; shepherd, matsushima, martin & clark, 1994; tykocinski, shepherd & clark', 1997). i die doel met psigoakoestiese eksperimente is om die waarneembare effek van insette na die gehoorstelsel te kwantifiseer. psigoakoestiese hoeveelhede soos die toonhoogte of luidheid van 'n klank is persoonafhanklik en kan nie eenduidig en direk gemeet word nie. psigo-j akoestiese hoeveelhede word dus statisties bepaal nadat' proefpersone 'n voldoende aantal herhalings van 'n ekspe-1 riment voltooi het om 'n statisties beduidende resultaat te lewer. deur die psigoakoestiek van akoestiese en elektriese stimulasie te vergelyk (j.j. hanekom, 2000; shannon, 1993; zeng & shannon, 1992) kan meer geleer word oor die seinverwerking wat die gehoorstelsel doen en oor hoe om te werk te gaan om beter inplantings te ontwikkel. psigoakoestiese navorsing kan direk lei tot verbeterde algoritmes (clark, 1996) en verbeterde programmering van bestaande inplantings (hanekom & shannon, 1996). navorsing moet multidissipliner wees die bespreking hierbo wys dat multidissiplinere navorthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die bydrae van basiese navorsing in kliniese toepassings met verwysing na kogleere inplantings 45 singspanne nodig is om probleme effektief aan te spreek en nuwe tegnologie te ontwikkel. natuurwetenskaplikes soos ingenieurs, fisioloe, fisici, wiskundiges en dierkundiges beskik oor die eksperimentele en analitiese gereedskap wat nodig is om die suiwer wetenskaplike en tegnologiese aspekte ter sprake by basiese navorsing op die gebied van kogleere inplantings aan te spreek. elke lid van 'n navorsingspan kan nie 'n kundige wees op elke tersaaklike gebied nie. ingenieurs het 'n goeie teoretiese en tegnologiese onderbou en kan fokus op modellering, die ontwikkeling van meetgereedskap en tegnologie. fisioloe kan weer fisiologiese parameters meet op selle of diere in die laboratorium. kliniese en basiese navorsing het verskillende (maar aanverwante) doelwitte. in 'n multidissiplinere span word beide tipes navorsing gedoen, en al die lede van die span kan betrokke wees by alle navorsingsprojekte, maar tipies word kliniese en basiese navorsing afsonderlik bestuur deur 'n klinikus en 'n natuurwetenskaplike onderskeidelik. wat is reeds bereik met basiese navorsing? enkele voorbeelde word hier gegee van die impak van basiese navorsing op die ontwikkeling van kogleere inplantings. dit is in psigoakoestiese eksperimente bepaal dat die frekwensie waarteen gestimuleer word op 'n enkele elektrode net gediskrimineer kan word tot omtrent 300 hz, maar dat deur die plek van stimulasie te verskuif, 'n baie groter perseptuele verandering in frekwensie verkry kan word as 300 hz (dorman, smith, smith & parkin, 1994; shannon, 1983). ander navorsing het gewys dat die elektrodes gerangskik kan word in tonotopiese volgorde (dorman, smith, dunnavant, parkin & dankowski, 1990; nelson, van tasell, schroder, soli & levine, 1995). daarom is besluit om frekwensie te kodeer as plek van stimulasie in al die bestaande multikanaalinplantings (clark, 1996; kessler, 1999). in 'n verdere lyn van navorsing is aangetoon dat die elektries-gestimuleerde gehoorstelsel net so goed of beter vaar as die normale ι gehoorstelsel met take wat tydre/ solusie meet (shannon, 1983; shannon, 1989; van wieringen & wouters, 1999). hierdie lyn van navorsing het gelei tot strategiee wat daarop gemik is om die tydpatrone in spraak so getrou as moontlik in stimulasiegolfvorms na te maak, bv. die cis strategie (loizou, 1999). wat is huidige navorsingsvrae? ι daar is 'n te groot aantal navorsingsvrae wat tans aangespreek word om almal hier te noem. enkele belangrike vrae word genoem. kan stroomverspreiding rondom elektrodekontakte beperk word deur beter elektrode-ontwerpe (cords, reuter, issing, sommer, kuzma & lenarz, 2000)? hoeveel interaksie is daar tussen neurale kanale wat met verskillende elektrodes gestimuleer word (fu, 1997; hanekom & shannon, 1998)? is kanaalinteraksie belangrik vir spraakverstaanbaarheid (fu & shannon, 1999a; fu & shannon, 1999b)? is dit plekkodering of tydkodering, of 'n kombinasie van beide die meganismes waarmee die gehoorstelsel frekwensie kodeer (j.j. hanekom, 2000; moller, 1999)? hoeveel elektrodes is nodig vir effektiewe oordraging van inligting aan die elektriesgestimuleerde gehoorstelsel (fishman, shannon & slattery, 1997)? hoe belangrik is die relatiewe tydverskille waarmee elektries gestimuleer word op die verskillende kanale (carlyon, geurts & wouters, 2000)? waar presies word die elektriese gestimuleerde seine gegenereer (javel & shepherd, 2000)? wat is die invloed van die stimulasiegolfvorm se parameters op die klank wat waargeneem word (chatterjee, fu & shannon, 2000)? hierdie is maar 'n klein uittreksel uit vrae, waarvan die antwoorde kan lei tot verbeterings in kogleere inplantings. gevolgtrekking om die kogleere inplanting-navorsingsveld volledig aan te spreek is dit noodsaaklik dat basiese navorsing in parallel met kliniese navorsing gedoen moet word. kliniese navorsing konsentreer op die habilitering en rehabilitering van die dowe persoon, terwyl basiese navorsing gemik is op uitbreiding van die kennis en begrip van die werking van die normale en elektries-gestimuleerde gehoorstelsel sowel as die ontwikkeling en verbetering van die kogleere inplantingtegnologie. basiese navorsing behoort modelgebaseerd te wees. wiskundige modelle vorm 'n belangrike deel van die meetgereedskap van die navorser. buiten hul waarde om bestaande kennis op te som, dra modelle by tot begrip van die werking van die stelsel (bv. die gehoorstelsel). modelle help ook om gapings in die bestaande kennis bloot te le, hetsy dit in onbekende parameterwaardes of in begrip van die werking van die stelsel le. basiese navorsing het reeds gelei tot kliniese toepassings waaronder die kogleere inplanting een van die uitstaande suksesse is. 'n verskeidenheid van moeilike navorsingsprobleme is egter onopgelos en sal georkestreerde pogings van multidissiplinere navorsingspanne benodig om die volgende kwantumsprong in inplantingtegnologie te maak. bronnelys allen, j. b. (1985). cochlear modeling. ieee acoustics, speech and signal processing magazine, january, 3-29. andreef, a. m., gersuni, g. v. & volokhov, a. a. (1934). electrical stimulation of the hearing organ. journal of physiology ussr, 17, 546-559. bower, j. m. (1990). reverse engineering of the nervous system: an anatomical, physiological, and computer-based approach. an introduction to neural and electronic networks. academic press. bruce, i. c., irlicht, l. s., white, m. w., o'leary, s. j., dynes, s., javel, e. & clark, g. m. (1999). a stochastic model of the electrically stimulated auditory nerve: single-pulse response. ieee transactions on biomedical engineering, 46(6), 617-629. bruce, i. c., white, m. w., irlicht, l. s., o'leary, s. j. & clark, g. m. (1999). effects of stochastic neural activity in a model predictive intensity perception with cochlear implants: lowrate stimulation. ieee transactions on biomedical engineering, 46, 1393-1404. brugge, j. f. (1992). an overview of central auditory processing. in popper, a. n. and r.r.fay, the mammalian auditory pathway: neurophysiology new york: springer-verlag. brummer, s. b. & turner, m. j. (1975). electrical stimulation of the nervous system: the principle of safe charge injection with noble metal electrodes. bioelectrochemistry and bioenergetics, 2, 13-25. carlyon, r. p., geurts, l. & wouters, j. (2000). detection of small across-channel timing differences by cochlear implantees. hearing research, 141, 140-154. chatterjee, m., fu, q.-j. & shannon, r. v. (2000). effects of phase duration and electrode separation on loudness growth in cochlear implant listeners. journal of the acoustical society of america, 107(3), 1637-1644. clark, g. m. (1993). the development of the nucleus multipledie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 tania hanekom & johan j. hanekom channel cochlear implant. australian journal of otolaryngology, 1(4), 310-316. clark, g. m. (1996). electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve: the coding of frequency, the perception of pitch and the development of cochlear implant speech processing strategies for profoundly deaf people. clinical and experimental pharmacology and physiology, 23, 766-776. clark, g. m., shepherd, r. k., patrick, j. f., black, r. c. & ibng, y. c. (1983). design and fabrication of the banded electrode array. annals of the new york academy of sciences, 405, 191201. clark, g. m., shute, s. α., shepherd, r. k. & carter, t. d. (1995). cochlear implantation: osteogenesis, electrode-tissue impedance, and residual hearing. annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology supplement (united states), 166, 40-42. cords, s. m., reuter, g., issing, p. r., sommer, α., kuzma, j. & lenarz, t. (2000). a silastic positioner for a modiolus-hugging position of intracochlear electrodes: electrophysiologic effects. american journal of otology, 21, 212-217. donaldson, n. d. n. & donaldson, p. ε. k. (1986). when are actively balanced ('lilly') stimulating pulses necessary in a neurological prosthesis? ii ph changes; noxious products; electrode corrosion; discussion. medical and biological engineering and computing, 24, 50-56. donaldson, p. ε. k. (1988). technical note: testing hermetic seals of microelectronic packages. journal of medical engineering and technology, 12(1), 26-27. donaldson, ρ ε. k., donaldson, n. d. n. & brindley, g. s. (1985). life of pt and pt-ir stimulating electrodes in neurological prostheses. medical and biological engineering and computing, 23, 84-86. donnelly, m. j., cohen, l. t., xu, j., xu, s. a. & clark, g. m. (1995). investigations on a curved intracochlear array. annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology supplement (united states), 166, 409-412. dorman, m. f., smith, l., dunnavant, g., parkin, j. & dankowski, k. (1990). pitch scaling and speech understanding by patients who use the ineraid cochlear implant. ear and hearing, 11(4), 310-315. dorman, m. f., smith, m., smith, l. & parkin, j. l. (1994). the pitch of electrically presented sinusoids. journal of the acoustical society of america, 95(3), 1677-1679. ehret, g. (1997). the auditory midbrain, a "shunting yard" of acoustical information processing. in ehret, g. and r.romand, the central auditory system new york: oxford university press. evans, e. f. (1978). place and time coding of frequency in the peripheral auditory system: some physiological pros and cons. audiology, 17, 369-420. finley, c. c., wilson, b. s. & white, m. w. (1990). models of neural responsiveness to electrical stimulation. in miller, j. m. and f.a.spelman, cochlear implants. models of the electrically stimulated ear new york: springer-verlag. fishman, p. m., shannon, r. v. & slattery, w. h. (1997). speech recognition as a function of the number of electrodes used in the speak cochlear implant speech processor. journal of speech and hearing research, 40, 1201-1215. frankenhaeuser, b. (1956). a method for recording resting and action potentials in the isolated myelinated nerve fibre of the frog. journal of physiology, 135, 550-559. frijns, j. η. m., de snoo, s. l. & schoonhoven, r. (1995). potential distributions and neural excitation patterns in a rotationally symmetric model of the electrically stimulated cochlea. in frijns, j. η. m., cochlear implants. a modelling approach den haag: cip-data koninklijke bibliotheek. fu q.-j. , chatterjee, m., hanekom, j. j. , shannon, r. v., zeng, f.-g. (1997). comparison of electrode interaction measures in multichannel cochlear implants. poster presentation at 1997 conference on implantable auditory prostheses, asilomar, ca, august 16-20. fu, q.-j. & shannon, r. v. (1999a). effects of electrode configuration and frequency allocation on vowel recognition with the nucleus-22 cochlear implant. ear and hearing, 20(4), 332-344. fu, q.-j. & shannon, r. v. (1999b). effects of electrode location and spacing on phoneme recognition with the nucleus-22 cochlear implant. ear and hearing, 20(4), 321-331. fujita, s. & ito, j. (1999). ability of nucleus cochlear implantees to recognize music. annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology, 108, 634-640. gabbiani, f. & koch, c. (1996). coding of time-varying signals in spike trains of integrateand-fire neurons with random threshold. neural computation, 8, 44-66. galvani, l. (1791). de viribus electricitatis in motu muscalari, commentarius. de bononiensi scientiarum et artium institute atque academia, 7, 363-418. girzon, g. (1987). investigation of current flow in the inner ear daring electrical stimulation of intracochlear electrodes. ms thesis in ee&cs, mit, cambridge, massachusetts. greenwood, d. d. (1990). a cochlear frequency-position function for several species 29 years later. journal of the acoustical society of america, 87(6), 2592-2605. hambrecht, f. t. (1985). principles for building biocompatible devices: safety considerations. in schindler, r. a. and m.m.merzenich, cochlear implants new york: raven press. hanekom, j. j. (2000). what do cochlear implants teach us about the encoding of frequency in the auditory system? south african journal of communication disorders, 47. hanekom, j. j. & kriiger, j. j. (2001). a model of frequency discrimination with optimal processing of auditory nerve spike intervals. hearing research, 151(1-2), 188-204. hanekom, j. j. & shannon, r. v. (1996). place pitch discrimination and speech recognition in cochlear implant users. the south african journal of communication disorders, 43, 2740. hanekom, j. j. & shannon, r. v. (1998). gap detection as a measure of electrode interaction in cochlear implants. journal of the acoustical society of america, 104(4), 2372-2384. hanekom, t. (2001). three-dimensional spiraling finite element model of the electrically stimulated cochlea. ear and hearing, in druk. hanekom, t. & hanekom, j. j. (1998). on the design of implantable electrodes for electrical nerve stimulation. south african journal of science, 94, 307-310. hanekom. t., hanekom, j. j. & marais, p. (1998). actively deposited ceramic layers as insulation on thin implantable electrode conductors. south african journal of science, 94, 310-311. hirsch, h. g. (1993). intelligibility improvements of noisy speech for people with cochlear implants. speech communication, 12, 261-266. hodgkin, a. l. & huxley, a. f. (1952). a quantitative description of membrane current and its application to conduction and excitation in nerve. journal of physiology, 117, 500-544. house, w. f. & urban, j. (1973). long term results of electrode implantation and electronic stimulation of the cochlea in man. annals of oto-rhino-laryngology, 82(4), 504-516. huang, c. q. & shepherd, r. k. (1999). reduction in excitability of the auditory nerve following electrical stimulation at high stimulus rates. iv. effects of stimulus intensity. hearing research, 132, 60-68. javel, e. (1990). acoustic and electrical encoding of temporal information. in miller, j. m. and f.a.spelman, cochlear implants: models of the electrically stimulated ear new york: springerverlag. javel, e. & shepherd, r. k. (2000). electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve. iii. response initiation sites and temporal fine structure. hearing research, 140, 45-76. johnson, p. f. & hench, l. l. (1977). an in vitro analysis of metal electrodes for use in the neural environment. brain, behav.evol., 14, 23-45. kessler, d. k. (1999). the clarion multi-strategy cochlear implant.' annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology, 108, 8-16. kou, b. s., shipp, d. b. & nedzelski, j. m. (1994). subject benefits reported by adult nucleus 22-channel cochlear implant users. journal of otolaryngology (canada), 23(1), 8-14. . lauridsen, o., gunthersen, c., bonding, p. & tos, m. (1982). experiments with a thinfilm multichannel electrode for cochlear implantation. acta otolaryngologica, 95, 219-226. lenhart, e. (1992). biocompatibility of cochlear implants. eur. arch. otorhinolaryngol. suppl. (germany), 13(2), 223-233. loizou, p. c. (1999). signal-processing techniques for cochlear implants. ieee engineering in medicine and biology, 18(3), 34-46. mccreery, d. b., agnew, w. f., yuen, t. g. h. & bullara, l. a. (1992). damage in peripheral nerve from continuous electrical stimulation: comparison of two stimulus waveforms. medical and biological engineering and the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die bydrae van basiese navorsing in kliniese toepassings met verwysing na kogleere inplantings 47 computing, 30, 109-114. miller, c. α., abbas, p. j., rubinstein, j. t., robinson, β. k., matsuoka, a. j. & woodworth, g. g. (1998). electrically evoked compound action potentials of guinea pig and cat: responses to monopolar, monophasic stimulation. hearing research, 119, 142-154. moller, a. r. (1999). review of the roles of temporal and place coding of frequency in speech discrimination. acta otolaryngology (stockholm), 119, 424-430. moore, b. c. j. & sek, a. (1996). detection of frequency modulation at low modulation rates: evidence for a mechanism based on phase locking. journal of the acoustical society of america, 100(4), 2320-2331. national institutes of health (1995). cochlear implants in adults and children. nih consensus statement, may 15-17,13(2), 130. nelson, d. α., van tasell, d. j., schroder, a. c., soli, s. & levine, s. (1995). electrode ranking of "place pitch" and speech recognition in electrical hearing. journal of the acoustical society of america, 98(4), 1987-1999. rattay, f. (1999). the basic mechanism for the electrical stimulation of the nervous system. neuroscience, 89, 335-346. rattay, f. & aberham, m. (1993). modeling axon membranes for functional electrical stimulation. ieee transactions on biomedical engineering, 40(12), 1201-1209. rattay, f., leao, r. n. & felix, h. (2000). a model of the electrically excited human cochlear neuron. ii. influence of the 3dimensional cochlear structure on neural excitability. hearing research. richards, a. m. (1976). basic experimentation in psychoacoustics. baltimore: university park press. rubinstein, j. t., soma, m. & spelman, f. a. (1985). mixed boundary value problems in the implanted cochlea: an analytical model of a cylindrical banded electrode array. ieee seventh annual conference of the engineering in medicine and biology society, 1120-1123. rubinstein, j. t„ wilson, b. s., finley, c. c. & abbas, p. j. (1999). pseudospontaneous activity: stochastic independence of auditory nerve fibers with electrical stimulation. hearing research, 127, 108-118. schwarz, j. r. & eikhof, g. (1987). na currents and action potentials in rat myelinated nerve fibres at 20 and 30 degrees c. pfliigers archiv, 409, 569-577. seldon, h. l., dahm, m. c„ clark, g. m. & crowe, s. (1994). silastic with polyacrylic acid filler: swelling properties, biocompatibility and potential use in cochlear implants. biomaterials, 15, 1161-1169. shannon, r. v. (1983). multichannel electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve in man. i. basic psychophysics. hearing research, 11, 157-189. shannon, r. v. (1989). detection of gaps in sinusoids and pulse / trains by patients with cochlear implants. journal of the acoustical society of america, 85(6), 2587-2592. shannon, r. v. (1993). quantitative comparison of electrically and acoustically evoked auditory perception: implications for the location of perceptual mechanisms. progress in brain research, 97, 261-269. shepherd, r. k., hatsushika, s.-i. & clark, g. m. (1993). electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve. the effect of electrode position on neural excitation. hearing research, 66, 108120. shepherd, r. k., matsushima, j., martin, r. l. & clark, g. m. (1994). cochlear pathology following chronic electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve: ii. deafened kittens. hearing research, 81(1-2), 150-166. shepherd, r. k., murray, μ. t„ houghton, μ. e. & clark, g. m. (1985). scanning electon microscopy of chronically stimulated platinum intracochlear electrodes. biomaterials, 6, 237-242. siebert, w. m. (1970). frequency discrimination in the auditory /^t58(5ar723p7e30odicity m e c h a n i s m s ? proceedings of the simmons f β mongeon, c. s„ lewis, w. r. & huntington, d. a. (1964). electrical stimulation of acoustical nerve and inferior colliculus: results in man. archives of otolaryngology, 79, 559-567. soma, m. (1986). reliability of implantable electronic devices. two case studies. ieee transactions on reliability r-35 483487. soma, m. (1990). a package design technique for size reduction of implantable bioelectronic systems. ieee transactions on biomedical engineering, 37, 482-488. stypulkowski, p. h. & van den honert, c. (1984). physiological properties of the electrically stimulated auditory nerve. i. compound action potential. hearing research, 14, 205-223. treaba, c. g., xu, j., xu, s. a. & clark, g. m. (1995). precurved electrode array and insertion tool. annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology supplement (united states), 166, 438-441. tye-murray, n., tyler, r. s., woodworth, g. g. & gantz, b. j. (1992). performance over time with nucleus or ineraid cochlear implant. ear and hearing, 13(3), 200-209. tykocinski, m., shepherd, r. k. & clark, g. m. (1997). reduction in excitability of the auditory nerve following electrical stimulation at high stimulus rates. ii. comparison of fixed amplitude with amplitude modulated stimuli. hearing research, 112, 147-157. van den honert, c. & stypulkowski, p. h. (1984). physiological properties of the electrically stimulated auditory nerve. ii. single fiber recordings. hearing research, 14, 225-243. van den honert, c. & stypulkowski, p. h. (1987a). single fiber mapping of spatial excitation patterns in the electrically stimulated auditory nerve. hearing research, 29, 195-206. van den honert, c. & stypulkowski, p. h. (1987b). temporal response patterns of single auditory nerve fibers eliceted by periodic electrical stimuli. hearing research, 29, 207-222. van noordt, r. & black, μ. m. (1981). silicone rubbers for medical applications. in williams, d. f., biocompatibility of clinical implant materials: vol ii florida: crc press inc. van wieringen, a. & wouters, j. (1999). gap detection in singleand multiple-channel stimuli by laura cochlear implantees. journal of the acoustical society of america, 106(4), 19251939. verveen, a. a. (1962). axon diameter and fluctuation in excitability. acta morphol.neerl.scand., 5, 79-85. volta, a. (1800). letter to sir joseph banks, march 20,1800. on electricity excited by the mere contact of conducting substances of different kinds. philisophical transactions of the royal society, 90, 403-431. white, j. α., rubinstein, j. t. & kay, a. r. (2000). channel noise in neurons. trends in neurosciences, 23, 131-137. zeng, f.-g., fu, q.-j. & morse, r. p. (2000). human hearing enhanced by noise. brain research, 869, 251-255. zeng, f.-g. & shannon, r. v. (1992). loudness balance between electric and acoustic stimulation. hearing research, 60, 231235. zeng, f.-g. & shannon, r. v. (1999). psychophysical laws revealed by electric hearing. neuroreport, 10, 1931-1935. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) time to think of a think about working in the uk s p e e c h a n d l a n g u a g e t h e r a p i s t s a n d a u d i o l o g i s t s quality locums are looking for quality personnel in all grades and specialities for work in the uk. eligibility for a visa or work permit would be an advantage, but even if you are not eligible w e would still like to hear from you a s w e may be able to help. quality locums are the largest independent medical, c a r e and education agency in the uk and w e have branches in south africa and australia. w e need medical staff of all s p e c i a l i t i e s , s o c i a l w o r k e r s a n d t e a c h e r s urgently to fill full and part time positions throughout great britain and ireland. w e are experts at helping you to take advantage of the^>pportunities in the uk. w h y not call one of our managers today for an informal discussion. s o n j a l e w i s q u a l i t y l o c u m s c a p e t o w n re.: 0 2 1 4 6 2 5 3 5 7 fax: 021 4625390 email: qualitylocums@worldonline.co.za matt w a g n e r q u a l i t y l o c u m s d u r b a n tel: 0 3 1 4 6 9 2 0 9 8 email wagner@yebo.co.za the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mailto:qualitylocums@worldonline.co.za mailto:wagner@yebo.co.za cpd december 2011 1. true (a) or false (b): literacy can be defined as one’s ability to read and speak. 2. true (a) or false (b): literacy skills of south african grade 5 learners are better than those of learners in neighbouring countries. 3. true (a) or false (b): language problems of children learning in their l2 have the most significant impact on their achievement in mathematics. 4. true (a) or false (b): the noise level in a classroom has little or no impact on learners’ reading skills. 5. true (a) or false (b): teachers identified limited reading and writing opportunities as a barrier to learning written language. 6. true (a) or false (b): teacher-learner ratios can affect reading comprehension of learners. 7. true (a) or false (b): the home/social community can influence learners’ academic success. 8. true (a) or false (b): the acquisition of listening skills is important in acquiring phonological awareness. 9. true (a) or false (b): phonological awareness is a predictor of reading and writing success. 10. true (a) or false (b): classroom instruction in the home language is important during the foundation phase of schooling. 11. true (a) or false (b): the majority of teachers in the wium & louw study used english as the language for learning and teaching in classrooms. 12. true (a) or false (b): currently 30% of slts in south africa have an african language as first language. 13. true (a) or false (b): rhyming is a common occurrence in african languages. 14. true (a) or false (b): learning and teaching in a second language can have a negative emotional impact on the learner and teacher. 15. true (a) or false (b): language factors are the only contributors to academic success. 16. true (a) or false (b): academic language skills are usually acquired through casual conversation. 17. true (a) or false (b): in south africa, sufficient attention is paid to educational linguistics. 18. true (a) or false (b): white paper 6 (department of education, 2011) omitted the role of slts in the educational setting. 19. true (a) or false (b): parental beliefs about literacy can have an impact on writing skill development. 20. true (a) or false (b): teachers’ skills to facilitate language development in the classroom can be enhanced by cpd activities. special edition on education cpd questionnaires must be completed online via www.cpdjournals.co.za. after submission you can check the answers and print your certificate. the south african journal of communication disorders sajcd 2 7 'η vergelyking van die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die simptome van persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie a. van der merwe, d.phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria r.j. grimbeek, m.sc (pretoria) departement statistiek universiteit van pretoria opsomming gegewens oor die individuele simptome van persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie is van teoretiese belang vir die studie van verbale apraksie. deur die vergelyking van individuele simptome kan liggewerp word op simptoombeelde oftipes verbale apraksie en op die aard van die afwyking by verworwe verbale apraksie en verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie. in hierdie ondersoek wat deel uitmaak van die groter ondersoek waarin die effek van variasie in kontekstuele faktore op verbaal apraktiese spraak nagegaan word (van der merwe, uys, loots en grimbeek, 1987; 1988; vander merwe, uys,loots, grimbeek en jansen, 1989) is die simptome van vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en een per soon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie onderling vergelyk. die ouditiefwaarneembarefoute en afwykings in stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur van die proefpersone is vergelyk. die resultate toon onder andere aan dat diefrekwensie van voorkoms van sekere simptome aanleidinggee tot individuele simptoombeelde, maar dat al die proefpersone die hoe voorkoms van simptome wat aanduidend is van die aard van die afwyking gemeen het. die proefpersoo'n met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie het al die simptome gemeen met die groep, maar vertoon minder afwykings in temper ale vloei en uitingduur en meerklankvervangings. die teoretiese implikasies van die resultate word bespreek. summary data on the individual symptoms of patients with acquired apraxia of speech and developmental apraxia of speech is of theoretical significance in the study of this disorder. a comparison of individual symptoms may shed light on error patterns in apraxia of speech, the possibility of types of apraxia ofspeechand on the nature of the disorder. in this study which was part of the wider investigation into the effect of variation in contextual factors on apraxia of speech (van der merwe, uys, loots and grimbeek, 1987; 1988; van der merwe, uys, loots, grimbeek and jansen, 1989) the symptoms of four patients with acquired apraxia of speech and one subject with developmental apraxia of speech were compared. the auditorily perceived symptoms and the deviations in voice onset time, vowel duration and utterance duration of all the subjects were compared. the results indicated that the frequency of occurrence of certain symptoms created individual error patterns but also that all subjects had the high occurrence of symptoms which reflect the nature of the disorder in common. the subject with developmental apraxia of speech had 'all symptoms in common with the other subjects but exhibited less deviancy in the temporal flow of speech and in the duration of the utterance.^he presented with more sound substitutions than did the acquired group. the theoretical implications of the results are discussed. ι tydens die navorsing oor die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op ouditief waarneembare foute, stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur by verbale apraksie (van der merwe, uys, loots & grimbeek, 1987; 1988; van der merwe, uys, loots, grimbeek en jansen, 1989) was die analises gebaseer op groepresultate alhoewel aandag ook subjektief geskenk is aan die gedrag van individue binne die groep. die data van die individuele proefpersone is egter ook statisties onderling vergelyk en in die huidige artikel word hierdie resultate aangebied. analise van die individuele resultate was belangrik aangesien dit bekend is dat alle apraktiese persone nie alle simptome gemeen het nie (wertz, la pointe & rosenbek, 1984) en ook omdat 'n proefpersoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie (voa) by die eksperimentele groep ingesluit was. gegewens oor die individuele simptome van proefpersone met verworwe verbale apraksie ( w a ) en voa is van teoretiese belang vir die studie van verbale apraksie. diagnostiese kriteria gegrond op sekere kenmerkende eienskappe van vva is vir die eerste keer geformuleer en in navorsing gei'mplimenteer deur kent en rosenbek (1983). hulle noem die volgende eienskappe van w a as kriteria: worstelende en soekende probeer-en-tref artikulasiebewegings en pogings tot selfkorreksie; disprosodie met geen verlengde periodes van normale ritme, klem en intonasie nie; foutonkonstantheid by herhaalde produksies van dieselfde eenheid; opsigtelike probleme in die inisiering van uitinge. dieselfde kriteria (tesame met ander kriteria wat die teenwoordigheid van afasie en disartrie uitskakel) is vir die seleksie van proefpersone in die huidige ondersoek gebruik. dit was reeds by seleksie duidelik dat nie alle simptome by al die proefpersone voorkom nie. die moontlikheid van verskillende tipes die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykins, vol. 37 1990 s a s 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 a. van der merwe & r.j. grimbeek verbale apraksie is reeds van melding gemaak in die literatuur (rosenbek, kent & la pointe, 1984; deutsch, 1984). die huidige ondersoek kan moontlik 'n bydrae maak tot kennis van individuele foutpatrone van persone wat almal as verbaal aprakties gediagnoseer kan word. in die huidige ondersoek is daar ook vir die eerste keer sover bekend 'n sistematiese vergelyking in navorsing getref tussen die simptome van persone met w a en 'n kind wat gediagnoseer is as verbaal aprakties. die blote bestaan van voa word soms nog betwyfel (guyette en diedrich, 1981; aram, 1984). die meeste vooraanstaande navorsers erken egter wel dat verbale apraksie by die kind kan voorkom (wertz, et al, 1984; yoss & darley, 1974). die belangrikste struikelblok in die beskrywing van sodanige diagnostiese groep is die gebrek aan diagnostiese kriteria wat hoofsaaklik te wyte is aan onvoldoende navorsing en die beperkte siening van verbale apraksie wat hoofsaaklik gegrond is op die simptome van die verworwe vorm. die verskille tussen individuele kinders met voa en die gekombineerde voorkoms met ander probleme veral by die meer algemeen breinbeseerde kind, bemoeilik die diagnose en beskrywing van voa verder. jarelange verkenning in die praktyk dui op die bestaan van minstens drie groepe kinders wat as suiwer verbaal-aprakties beskryf kan word (van der merwe, 1985). die eerste is die groep wat nie-verbaal is, maar met goeie reseptiewe taal. die tweede is 'n groep wat spraakagtige uitings vertoon soos slegs vokaalproduksie en die derde groep kommunikeer wel verbaal, maar is onverstaanbaar. beide hierdie groepe beskik ook oor goeie reseptiewe taal vermoe. verdere navorsing word tans onderneem om hierdie groepe te verifieer. die kind wat in die huidige ondersoek ingesluit is, het by aanvanklike diagnose in groep 1, die nie-verbale groep geval. afgesien van die vraag of daar wel so 'n afwyking soos voa is, bestaan die vraag ook of die kern van die probleem by die kongenitale of verworwe vorms soortgelyk is. guyette en diedrich (1981) verwys in hul bespreking na die rasionaal dat dieselfde benadering in behandeling geskik sal wees vir beide w a en voa indien die kern van die probleem ooreenstem. sodanige waarneming is wel plaaslik gedoen. die terapieprogram vir voa wat daarop gemik is om die beplanning van spraak te fasiliteer en uit te bou is suksesvol bevind by aldrie bogenoemde tipes voa en ook by gevalle met suiwer w a (van der merwe, 1985). guyette en diedrich (1981:41) meld ook in hul oorsig van voa dat "it is believed that response to treatment is a strong argument in support of a diagnostic category". dit is die teoretiese uitgangspunt in hierdie artikel dat beide w a en voa afwykings in die vermoe van die brein is om spraak te beplan of te leer beplan (van der merwe, 1986). indien ooreenstemmende simptome wel gevind word by die proefpersone met w a en die persoon met voa sal dit nie alleen die aanvanklike diagnose van voa grootliks bevestig nie, maar dit sal ook 'n sterk aanduiding wees van die ooreenkoms in die onderliggende aard van die probleem. die beskrywing van die individuele foutpatrone van al die proefpersone sal ook bydra tot 'n beter gedefinieerde beeld van w a en voa. metode d o e l die doel van hierdie bepaalde komponent van die ondersoek is om die individuele verskille in die simptome van die proefpersone met verworwe verbale apraksie verder toe te lig en ookom te bepaal in watter opsigte die proefpersoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie verskil van of ooreenstem met die ander vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie. e k s p e r i m e n t e l e o n t w e r p die proefpersone, luisteraars, materiaal en prosedure van ondersoek is reeds volledig beskryf in die voorafgaande artikels (van der merwe et al, 1987, 1988, 1989) en slegs enkele belangrike aspekte sal hier herhaal word. die totale ondersoek het bestaan uit 'n perseptuele analise van die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute van persone met verbale apraksie en uit 'n akoestiese analise van die invloed van dieselfde faktore op stemaanvangstyd (sat), vokaalduur en uitingduur. die kontekstuele faktore wat nagegaan is bestaan uit 'n sistematiese variasie in die klankstruktuur (foneemstruktuur) en die artikulasie-eienskappe (motoriese kompleksiteit) van onsineenhede waarin hierdie eienskappe volkome gekontroleerd is. die kriteria vir samestelling van hierdie materiaal word volledig beskryf in van der merwe (1986). die materiaal bestaan uit agt eenhede in vyf klankstruktuurgroepe en die eenhede in elke klankstruktuurgroep word verdeel in vier artikulasie-eienskapgroepe (van der merwe, et al, 1987). al die proefpersone het hierdie veertig eenhede ses keer agtereenvolgend herhaal na 'n kort oefenperiode waarin elke eenheid eers selfstandig geproduseer moes word. die uitings was dus selfge'fnisieerd en nie direkte nabootsings nie. die proefpersone het wel die uitings in 'n geskrewe vorm voor hulle gehad ten einde geheueprobleme sover moontlik te beperk. tydens die eerste stadium van die ouditiewe analise van die data is 'n lys saamgestel van alle spraakfoute wat voorgekom het by die vyf proefpersone. selfs al sou net een van die proefpersone 'n sekere simptoom vertoon, is dit in die lys opgeneem. foutkategoriee is verder in samewerking met twee medeluisteraars saamgestel (van der merwe, 1987). p r o e f p e r s o n e i vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en een persoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie (voa) is in hierdie ondersoek gebruik. die volledige gegewens word aangegee in van der merwe et al, (1987). j ι die persoon met verbale ontwikkelirigsapraksie is aanvanklik slegs op 'n voorlopige grondslag ingesluit by die ondersoek vanwee die beperkte getal persone met 'n "suiwer" verworwe verbale apraksie wat opgespoor kon word. daar is later in samewerking met die statistikus besluit dat die resultate van hierdie proefpersoon in so 'n mate ooreenkom met die eksperimentele groep se resultate dat dit tesame daarmee verwerk kan word, 'n afgepaarde kontrolepersoon is ook vir die proefpersoon met voa geselekteer vir vergelyking van die akoesties geanaliseerde data. ten einde te verseker dat die proefpersoon met voa (proefpersoon 5) wel verbale apraksie vertoon, is die volgende kenmerkende eienskappe as seleksiekriteria gestel. — 'n diskrepansie tussen ekspressiewe en reseptiewe taalvermoe wat nie meer as ses maande agter sy chronologiese ouderdom is nie het voorgekom tydens die inisiele diagnose. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vergelyking van faktore op simptome van persone met verworwe verbaleen verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie 29 'n onvermoe om willekeurige spraakbewegings uit te voer met gevolglike nie-verbaliteit het voorgekom. kommunikasie het deur middel van gebare geskied. proefpersoon 5 is op die ouderdom van vier as 'n geval met voa gediagnoseeren hy het sedert daardie tyd verbale apraksieterapie (van der merwe, 1985) ontvang. ten tyde van die ondersoek het hy reeds goed verstaanbaar gekommunikeer in vyf tot seswoordsinne. ten spyte van die goeie vordering tydens die ses jaar van behandeling het hy steeds simptome van verbale apraksie vertoon en veral die lengte van woorde wat gebruik is, was nog beperk. vir die vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie is die kriteria van kent en rosenbek (1983) gestel. hierdie persone het onvlot spraakproduksie met probeer-en-tref artikulasiebewegings, pogings tot selfkorreksie, onkonstantheid in die foute by herhaalde produksie van dieselfde woord en disprosodie vertoon. hulle moes ook aan verdere kriteria voldoen om die teenwoordigheid van 'n gepaardgaande afasie of disartrie uit te skakel (van der merwe et al, 1987). a n a l i s e van d i e d a t a ten einde die mate van afwyking van die groepgemiddeld te bepaal, is similariteitsvektore (steyn, smit en du toit, 1984) vir die individuele proefpersone bereken. by die berekening van similariteitsvektore vir sat, vokaalduur en uitingsduur is die verskil tussen die gemiddelde foutwaardes van elke persoon en die groepgemiddeld bereken en die totaal van hierdie verskille is sodoende bekom. kleiner totale dui op kleiner afwykings vanaf die groepgemiddeld. by die berekening van similariteitsvektore vir ouditief waarneembare foute is die verskil tussen die rangnommer (bepaal op grond van frekwensie van voorkoms) van elke tipe fout van die groep en die rangnommer van elke tipe fout van die individu bereken. resultate ί m a t e van a f w y k i n g van die i n d i v i d u e l e p r o e f p e r s o n e v a n a f g r o e p g e m i d d e l d e s vir s a t , v o k a a l d u u r , u i t i n g d u u r en o u d i t i e f w a a r n e e m b a r e f o u t e in tabel 1 word die proefpersone gerangskik in volgorde van afwyking vanaf die groepgemiddeldes vir al die aspekte wat ondersoek is. persone wat eerste geplaas word wyk die meeste af van die groepgemiddeld en die wat laaste (vyfde) geplaas word, wyk die minste af. in die tweede gedeelte van die tabel word die finale rangorde van elke proefpersoon bepaal. die hoogste totaal toon die kleinste afwyking vanaf die groep en die kleinste totaal die grootste afwyking. die plasing van proefpersone by die verskillende aspekte wat ondersoek is, toon aan dat variasie in individuele simptoombeelde voorkom na gelang van die mate waarin 'n besondere aspek aangetas is. geen een van die proefpersone neem konstant die eerste of laaste of enige ander posisie in nie. proefpersone 1 en 2 wat in die finale rangorde die naaste aan die groepgemiddeld gepresteer het, vertoon nie konstant die kleinste verskil van die groep nie. proefpersoon 1 toon byvoorbeeld by vokaalduur die tweede grootste afwyking van die groep en proefpersoon 2 neem ook dieselfde posisie in ten opsigte van sat en uitingduur. tabel 1: proefpersone gerangskik in volgorde van afwyking vanaf die groepgemiddeldes vir sat, vokaalduur, uitingduur en ouditief waarneembare foute aspek wat ontleed is verskil meeste verskil minste van groep van groep 1 2 3 4 5 sat *pp 3 pp 2 pp 4 pp 5 pp 1 vokaalduur pp 4 pp 1 pp 5 pp 3 pp 2 uitingduur pp 5 pp 2 pp 3 pp 1 pp 4 ouditief waarneembare foute pp 5 pp 1 pp 2 pp 3 pp 4 proefpersone rangorde van proefpersoon binne die groep 1 2 3 4' 5 5 + 2 + 4 + 3 = 14x 2 + 5 + 2 + 5 = 14 1 + 4 + 3 + 3 = 1 1 3 + 1 + 5 + 3 = 12 4 + 3 + 1 + 1 = 9 1 — verskil minste 2 van groep 4 3 5 — verskil meeste van groep *pp = proefpersoon χ die persoon met die kleinste totaal het die meeste afgewyk van die groep proefpersoon 4 wat telkens tydens die vorige besprekings uitgesonder is (van der merwe et al, 1987, 1988, 1989), verskil volgens hierdie analise nie sover van die groep as wat verwag is nie. in die finale rangorde word proefpersoon 4 derde geplaas. die grootste afwyking vertoon hy by vokaalduur terwyl uitingduur die minste van die groepgemiddeld verskil. segmentele duur was dus in die besonder versteur, maar totale duur van die uiting is dieselfde as die gemiddeld van die groep. oorgangtye tussen klanke was dus korter. proefpersone 2 en 4 vertoon onderskeidelik ten opsigte van sat die tweede en derde grootste afwyking van die groep omdat hulle weinig sat-foute gemaak het. proefpersoon 3 het daarenteen besonder baie sat-foute en wyk daarom die meeste af van die groepgemiddeld. proefpersoon 1 presteer die naaste aan die groepgemiddeld. die aard van die sat-foute wat wel voorgekom het was by al die proefpersone dieselfde, naamlik te grootpositiewe tellings wat veroorsaak dat stemhebbende klanke as stemloos geperseptueer word. die sat-foute is deur al die proefpersone onkonstant gemaak en daar is bevind dat hierdie foute nie konteks-sensitief is nie. die foute tree dus onverwags en onvoorspelbaar in. die aard van afwykings in vokaalduur en uitingduur was dieselfde by al die proefpersone. in vergelyking met die kontrolepersone het al vyf die proefpersone verlengde vokaalduur en uitingduur vertoon. statistiese analises het ook aangetoon dat vokaalduur en uitingduur konteks-sensitief is en dat duur toeneem namate die kompleksiteit en lengte van die uiting toeneem. 'n subjektiewe vergelyking van duurafwykings by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe (s) van elke proefpersoon toon aan dat al die proefpersone konteks-sensitiwiteit vertoon. (kyk tabelle 4 en 5 in van der merwe et al, 1989). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivy kings, vol 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 a. van der merwe & r.j. grimbeek die totale syfers (soos aangegee in tabel 1) waarmee al die proefpersone afwyk van die groep (9 tot 14) verskil minimaal en dit blyk dus dat nie een van die proefpersone radikaal van die groep verskil nie. die proefpersone vertoon dieselfde tipe simptome, maar individuele variasie in die mate waarin spesifieke aspekte aangetas is, kom voor. die finale rangordes in tabel 1 toon aan dat proefpersoon 5, die persoon met voa, die meeste verskil van die groep. dit is egter slegs ten opsigte van uitingduur en die ffekwensiepatroon van ouditief waarneembare foute wat proefpersoon 5 die meeste afwyk van die groep. wat sat aanbetref word proefpersoon 5 vierde geplaas en vertoon dus net 'n klein afwyking vanaf die groepgemiddeld. hierdie proefpersoon het ook soos die ander onkonstante sat-foute gemaak wat nie konteks-sensitief is nie en die vorm aanneem van te groot positiewe tellings. ten opsigte van vokaalduur neem proefpersoon 5 die derde posisie in en vertoon ook verlengde vokaalduur soos die persone met w a . vokaalduur is ook by proefpersoon 5 kontekssensitief. die enigste buitengewone resultaat was dat vokaalduur vir s5 wat 'n kvkvkvk-struktuur het, nie soos by die ander proefpersone meer afwykend was as vokaalduur van die ander klankstruktuurgroepe nie. 'n moontlike verklaring hiervoor is dat proefpersoon 5 geneig was om eenhede in s5 fonologies te verkort en spraakproduksie is dan aangepas by die korter en meer eenvoudige eenheid. proefpersoon 5 het net soos die ander proefpersone kontekssensitiwiteit in uitingduur vertoon. by s3 wat 'n kvkvkstruktuur het, het hy die grootste afwyking en die tweede grootste afwyking was by s5. klankstruktuurgroep 4 wat 'n kvk-struktuur het, vertoon die kleinste afwyking (kyk tabel 5 in van der merwe et al, 1989). hierdie patroon van kontekssensitiwiteit is dieselde as by die ander proefpersone. o u d i t i e f w a a r n e e m b a r e f o u t p a t r o n e van die i n d i v i d u e l e p r o e f p e r s o n e in tabel 2 word die frekwensie (en persentasie) van voorkoms vandie foutkategoriee by elke proefpersoon aangegee. uit die gegewens in hierdie tabel blyk dit dat die proefpersone alle foute gemeen het behalwe dat proefpersoon 1 geen foute gevolg deur selfkorreksie getoon het nie en proefpersoon 4 het geen veranderinge van struktuur getoon nie. die totale aantal foute toon aan dat wisselende hoeveelhede foute voorkom by die individuele proefpersone. dit verskaf 'n aanduiding van die graad van aantasting. ten einde 'n duideliker beeld te kry van individuele foutpatrone is die foutkategoriee gerangskik in dalende volgorde van voorkoms by elke proefpersoon. hierdie gegewens word aangegee in tabel 3. die rangskikking van foutkategoriee toon aan dat of distorsie (c) of afwykings in temporale vloei (d) die hoogste frekwensie tabel 2: die frekwensie (en persentasie) van voorkoms van die foutkategoriee by elke proefpersoon. foutkategoriee proefpersone a vervangings β verandering van struktuur c distorsie d ε afwyking in . _ , afwykings m temporale / , r , , . vlotheid vloei f g s e l f k o r r e k s i e m w y k i n f i n prosodie totale aantal foute 1 7 ( 1,6%) 27 ( 6,2%) 227 (52,0%) 169 (38,8%) 5 ( 1,2%) 0 ( 1 0%) ( 0,2%) 436 (100%) 2 11 ( 2,8%) 58 (14,6%) 94 (23,6%) 168 (42,2%) 8 ( 2,0%) 3 56 ( 0,8%) (14,0%) 398 (100%) 3 53 ( 8,5%) 74 (11,9%) 209 (33,5%) 158 (25,3%) 87 (14,0%) 14 29 ( 2,2%) ( 4,6%) 1 624 i (100%) 4 25 ( 4,0%) 0 ( o%) 57 ( 9,2%) 359 (58,0%) 15 ( 2,4%) 12 ( 152 1,9%) (24,5%) 1 , 620 ! (100%) 5 118 (27,4%) 82 (19,1%) 182 (42,4%) 38 ( 8,8%) 3 ( 0,7%) 3 4 ( 0,7%) ( 0,9%) 430 ' (100%) totaal 214 241 769 892 118 32 " 242 2508 tabel 3: rangskikking van foutkategoriee in dalende volgorde van voorkoms by elke proefpersoon -proefpersone rangskikking van foutkategoriee 1 c (52,0%) d (38,8%) β ( 6,2%) a ( 1,6%) ε ( 1,2%) g ( 0,2%) f ( 0%) 2 d (42,2%) c (23,6%) β (14,6%) g (14,0%) a ( 2,8%) ε ( 2,0%) v x { 0,8%) 3 c (33,5%) d (25,3%) ε (14,0%) β (11,9%) a ( 8,5%) g ( 4,6%) ' ' f ( 2,2%) 4 d (58,0%) g (24,5%) c ( 9,2%) a ( 4,0%) ε ( 2,4%) f .(/l9%) β ( 0%) s 5 c (42,4%) a (27,4%) β (19,1%) d ( 8, 8%) g ( 0,9%) ε ( 0,7%) f ( 0 , 7 % ) ^ / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vergelyking van faktore op simptome van persone met verworwe verbaleen verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie van voorkoms vertoon by al die proefpersone. by drie van die proefpersone neem een van hierdie foute ook die tweede plek in. dit is slegs proefpersone 4 en 5 wat nie hierdie patroon volg nie. by proefpersoon 4 neem distorsie wel die derde plek in. afwykings in vlotheid (e), foute gevolg deur selfkorreksie (f) en/of afwykings in prosodie (g) neem by al die proefpersone die voorlaaste en/of die laaste posisie in. slegs by proefpersoon 4 neem veranderinge in struktuur (b) die laaste posisie in. al die proefpersone vertoon dus ook ten opsigte van die laagste voorkoms van spesifieke foutkategoriee ooreenstemmende foutpatrone. foutkategoriee a, β en ε neem by die meeste proefpersone die middelposisies in. dit is egter slegs by proefpersoon 3 dat afwykings in vlotheid (e) die derde hoogste frekwensie van voorkoms vertoon. proefpersoon 3 se spraak was baie meer onvlot en worstelend as die spraak van die ander proefpersone. proefpersoon 4 vertoon ook 'n individuele foutpatroon wat hom onderskei van die ander proefpersone. vanwee die besondere aantasting van proefpersoon 4 neem afwykings in temporale vloei (d) en waarskynlik as gevolg daarvan afwykings in prosodie (g) onderskeidelik die eerste en tweede plekke in. hy is dus die enigste persoon by wie distorsie nie een van die eerste twee plekke inneem nie. uit die gegewens in tabel 2 word dit duidelik dat hierdie persoon baie meer foute in bogenoemde twee aspekte het (359 by d en 152 by g) as die ander proefpersone. hy was ook die enigste persoon wat nie veranderinge in struktuur (b) vertoon het nie en hy het ook weinig sat-foute gemaak. proefpersoon 4 het wel die laagste voorkoms van distorsie-foute, maar die foute wat wel voorgekom het was ook onkonstante distorsies soos teenwoordig by die ander proefpersone. die bepaling van similariteitsvektore op grond van foutfrekwensiepatrone (bylae c) en die rangskikking van proefpersone in volgorde van afwyking van die groep (tabel 1) toon aan dat proefpersoon 2 die minste verskil van die groep terwyl proefpersone 1, 3 en 4 gesamentlik die middelposisie inneem. die mate waarin hierdie drie' afwyk van die groep stem dus ooreeri. die resultate ten opsigte van die ouditief waarneembare foute bevestig dus die resultate van die akoestiese ontledings naamlik dat al die proefpersone die meeste van die foute gemeen het maar dat individuele foutpatrone of simptoombeelde voorkom na gelang van die mate waarin 'n besondere aspek aangetas is. by al die proefpersone vertoon afwykings in temporale vloei of distorsie die hoogste voorkoms. al die proefpersone het dus hierdie kenmerk gemeen. die rangskikking van proefpersone in volgorde van afwyking vanaf die groepgemiddeldes in tabel 1, toon verder ook aan dat proefpersoon 5 die meeste verskil van die groep ten opsigte van die frekwensiepatroon van ouditief waarneembare foute. die verrassendste verskynsel is egter dat hierdie proefpersoon met voa wel dieselfde simptome gemeen het met die ander proefpersone. die gegewens in tabel 2 toon aan dat hy foute in elke foutkategorie vertoon het. geen ander spraakfoute kom by hom voor nie omdat alle foute van al die proefpersone tydens die analise van die data aangeteken is. die rangskikking van foutkategoriee in tabel 3 toon aan dat proefpersoon 5 ook soos twee ander proefpersone die hoogste voorkoms van distorsiefoute vertoon. die drie simptome (g, ε en f) met die laagste voorkoms het hy ook gemeen met die ander proefpersone. die enigste groot verskille in frekwensie van voorkoms van ouditief waarneembare foute is die hoe voorkoms van vervangingsfoute (a) en die relatief lae voorkoms van afwykings in temporale vloei. uit tabel 2 blyk dit dat proefpersoon 5 , 1 1 8 vervangings vertoon het in vergelyking met die ander proefpersone wat gewissel het van 7 tot 53. hy vertoon ook meer veranderinge in die struktuur van die eenheid. dit is moontlik dat hierdie twee probleme saamhang want tydens die ondersoek het dit duidelik geword dat proefpersoon 5 nie die struktuur van s5-eenhede kon onthou ofproduseer nie. hy was geneig om hierdie eenhede fonologies te vereenvoudig en/of te verkort. die lae voorkoms van afwykings in die temporale vloei van spraak by proefpersoon 5 is in ooreenstemming met die akoestiese bepaling van uitingduur. proefpersoon 5 vertoon minder afwykend as die persone met w a ten opsigte van beide hierdie aspekte. d i e i n v l o e d van k l a n k s t r u k t u u r o p d i e g e t a l o u d i t i e f w a a r n e e m b a r e f o u t e van d i e i n d i v i d u e l e p r o e f p e r s o n e die totale aantal ouditief waarneembare foute van al die proefpersone by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe word aangegee in tabel 4. in hakies word die rangorde van elke klankstruktuur aangedui. dit blyk duidelik dat variasie in die aantal foute by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe voorkom. die laagste voorkoms van foute by al die proefpersone is by s4 wat 'n kvk-struktuur het. die lae voorkoms van foute is deels te wyte aan die korter struktuur wat minder foute toelaat, maar die akoestiese analises van byvoorbeeld die eerste vokaal van al die klankstrukture het aangetoon dat dit nie die oorwegende faktor is nie. die konteks van die kvk-struktuur het 'n ooreenstemmende invloed gehad op al die proefpersone. dit is interessant dat dit ook die geval was by proefpersoon 5 want tydens die fasilitasie van verbaliteit by hierdie geval met voa, het hy die produksie van kvk-strukture eers 'n paar jaar na die produksie van kvkv -strukture bemeester. tabel 4: totale getal ouditief waarneembare foute van die vyf proefpersone by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe klankstruktuur 1 proefpersone 2 3 4 5 1 95(3) 98(5) 132(3) 124(2) 75(3) 2 65(2) 84(2) 109(2) 145(4) 66(2) 3 119(5) 88(3) 151(4) 154(5) 92(4) 4 . 52(1) 35(1) 76(1) 61(1) 37(1) 5 105(4) 93(4) 156(5) 136(3) 160(5) totale aantal foute 436 398 624 620 430 dit blyk verder uit tabel 4 dat s3 of s5 tot die meeste foute aanleiding gegee het by al die proefpersone behalwe proefpersoon 2. by hierdie persoon het s5 wel die tweede hoogste voorkoms van foute vertoon. die redes vir die hoer getal foute by hierdie klankstrukture is vroeer reeds volledig bespreek (van der merwe et al, 1987,1988). binne die konteks van die huidige bespreking is die belangrike punt dat die proefpersone, ingeslote proefpersoon 5, oorwegend ooreenstemmend gereageer het op variasie in die klankstruktuur van die uiting. dit is interessant dat ook by proefpersoon 5, s3 wat slegs een klank langer is as si en s2 (kvkvk teenoor kvkv) soveel meer foute tot gevolg het. dit wil dus voorkom asof die toename in kompleksiteit van die uiting meer probleme in die beplanning van spraak te weeg bring by die proefpersone met w a maar ook by die proefpersoon met voa. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 a. van der merwe & r.j. grimbeek b e s p r e k i n g s l m p t o o m b e e l d e van p e r s o n e m e t v e r w o r w e v e r b a l e a p r a k s i e . die akoestiese-analise van sat, vokaalduur en uitingduur en die analise van ouditief waarneembare foute toon aan dat die proefpersone met w a die meeste van die groepe foute gemeen het, maar dat individuele simptoombeelde na vore kom na gelang van die varierende mate waarin verskillende aspekte aangetas is. die kollektie we voorkoms van sekere simptome lei dus tot die beeld van vaa, maar die besondere konstellasie van die simptome verskil skynbaar van persoon tot persoon. die analises het egter ook aangetoon dat beide klankdistorsie en afwykings in temporale vloei of een van hierdie twee groepe ouditief waarneembare simptome die hoogste frekwensie van voorkoms by al die proefpersone vertoon het. die hoe voorkoms van klankdistorsie by al die proefpersone bevestig die uitspraak van itoh en sasanuma (1984) dat klankdistorsie waarskynlik die kernsimptoom van verbale apraksie is. vroeer is distorsie nie gereken as 'n simptoom van verbale apraksie nie. dit is egter duidelik dat afwykings in die beplanning van spraak onder andere aanleiding kan gee tot onkonstante temporale en ruimtelike oorskreiding van die ekwivalensiegrense van spraakbewegings wat dan aanleiding gee tot klankdistorsie. die hoe voorkoms van afwykings in temporale vloei is ook in ooreenstemming met die aanvaarde aard van verbale apraksie. vloei van spraak word belemmer en simptome soos die afbakening van lettergrepe, stadige doelbewuste artikulasie en verlenging van klanke kom voor. die hoer frekwensie van voorkoms van sekere simptome by spesifieke persone gee aanleiding tot eiesoortige foutpatrone of simptoombeelde wat moontlik aanduidings is vanverskillende tipes w a . in die huidige ondersoek het minstens drie verskillende simptoombeelde by die persone met w a na vore gekom. proefpersoon 3 het groter afwykings in vlotheid as die ander proefpersone vertoon weens worstelende artikulasie. dit het voorgekom asof die inisiering van uitings vir haar besonder moeilik was. proefpersoon 4 daarenteen vertoon 'n hoe voorkoms van verlenging van die statiese periodes van artikulasie met baie min afwykings in vlotheid. dit het byna voorgekom asof sy spraakafwyking nader aan 'n disartrieseprobleem is as die van die ander proefpersone. die konstante distorsie en ander uiterlike tekens kenmerkend van disartrie was egter nie teenwoordig nie en die artikulasieverlenging was ook nie konstant teenwoordig nie. die subkortikale skade van hierdie persoon het moontlik aanleiding tot die besondere simptoombeeld gegee. kertesz (1984) het verbale apraksie by persone met subkortikale skade nagegaan en hy kom tot die slotsom dat die sogenaamde subkortikale sindroom grootliks ooreenkom met die kortikale sindroom. hy het by die subkortikale sindroom ook disartiese simptome gevind, maar het nie die konstantheid van die simptome nagegaan nie. proefpersoon 4 het egter kortikale en subkortikale skade gehad. proefpersoon 1 vertoon in teenstelling met bogenoemde twee persone die klassieke beeld van suiwer w a (itoh & sasanuma, 1984; wertz et al, 1984). proefpersoon 1 presenteer met die hoogste voorkoms van klankdistorsie en ook 'n hoe voorkoms van afwykings in temporale vloei terwyl sy 'n baie lae voorkoms en in baie gevalle die laagste voorkoms van die ander groepe ouditief waarneembare foute vertoon. die hoe voorkoms van klankdistorsie en afwykings in temporale vloei by hierdie besondere proefpersoon asook die feit dat een of beide van hierdie foute die hoogste voorkoms by al die ander proefpersone vertoon, is 'n sterk aanduiding dat hierdie groepe simptome die kenmerkende eienskappe van w a is en ook in aanmerking geneem moet word wanneer diagnostiese kriteria vir w a geformuleer word. navorsing met groter groepe is egter nodig om hierdie resultate te bevestig. v e r b a l e o n t w i k k e l i n g s a p r a k s i e as d i a g n o s t i e s e e n t i t e i t proefpersoon 5 wat op die ouderdom van vier jaar gediagnoseer is as 'n geval met voa en wat reeds tien jaar oud was ten tyde van die ondersoek, vertoon in die geheel gesien verrasende ooreenstemming met die proefpersone met w a . die similariteitsvektore toon aan dat hy wat die totale ondersoek betref die meeste van die groep verskil, maar wat die onderskeie aspekte betref verskil hy slegs ten opsigte van uitingduur en die frekwensiepatroon van ouditief waarneembare foute die meeste van die groep. hy het egter al die simptome gemeen met die groep. proefpersoon 5 vertoon soos die persone met w a onkonstante sat-foute wat ook die vorm aanneem van te groot positie we tellings, verlengde vokaalduur en verlengde uitingduur alhoewel laasgenoemde minder afwykend is as die uitingduur van die persone met w a . hy reageer ook dieselfde as die ander, proefpersone op variasie in die klankstruktuur van die uiting. wat die ouditief waarneembare foute betref het hy die groepe simptome met die hoogste en laagste voorkoms gemeen met die groep. die distorsiefoute wat by hom soos by twee ander proefpersone die hoogste voorkoms vertoon het, neem ook die vorm aan van onkonstante distorsies van klanke tydens herhaalde produksie van 'n uiting. die grootste verskille van die groep vertoon proefpersoon 5 ten opsigte van die hoe voorkoms van klankvervangings en die lae voorkoms van afwykings in temporale vloei. die grootste aantal klankvervangings was weens fonologiese vereenvoudiging van die langer eenhede (s5) wat moontlik verband hou met beperkte fonologiese geheue of 'n onvermoe om die lang uitinge te produseer. die feit dat dit onsineenhede was wat geproduseer moes word, speel waarskynlik ook 'n rol. dit is ook moontlik dat hierdie simptoom in samehang is met die totale beeld van vertraagde spraakontwikkeling. ι die lae voorkoms van afwykings in temporale vloei word bevestig deur die uitingduurmetings. ten opsigte van uitingduur vertoon proefpersoon 5 die grootste afwyking vanaf die groepgemiddeld omdat sy spraakspoed hoer is.'n moontlike verklaring vir hierdie verskynsel is dat alle spraak wat hy wel het doelbewus aangeleer is deur middel van die terapieprogram vir verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie (van der merwe, 1985). 'n belangrike grondbeginsel van die program is dat spraakuitinge doelbewus herhaaldelik geproduseer word sodat outomatisasie kan intree. alhoewel die proefpersone met w a dieselfde tipe behandeling ontvang het, is dit moontlik dat doelbewuste beplanning en inisiering van spraak in groter mate by hulle teenwoordig is en meer tyd in beslag neem. die afleiding kan nie sonder meer gemaak word dat alle kinders met voa vinniger spraakspoed as persone met w a vertoon nie omdat die spraak wat proefpersoon 5 het, die resultaat is van hierdie besondere benadering in behandeling. / die ooreenkoms in simptome van die persoon met voa en die ander proefpersone met w a bevestig nie alleen grootliks die bestaan van voa as 'n diagnostiese entiteit nie, maar ook die akkuraatheid van die aanvanklike diagnose van voa. by / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vergelyking van faktore op simptome van persone met verworwe verbaleen verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie 33 proefpersoon 5 as vierjarige kind het die teenwoordigheid van 'n probleem in die aanleer van die beplanning van spraak daartoe gelei dat hy nie-verbaal was ten spyte van goeie taalbegrip. dit wil dus voorkom asof die diagnostiese kriteria van groep 1 voa (van der merwe, 1985) as korrek aanvaar kan word. in die literatuur word geen oorweging daaraan geskenk of die moontlikheid genoem dat voa kan presenteer as nie-verbaliteit nie. die diagnostiese kenmerke wat beskryf word sentreer om die aard van artikulasiefoute en orale apraksie (macalusohayes, 1978; aram, 1984; yoss & darley, 1974) en geeneen van hierdie beskrywings het nog bygedra tot 'n oplossing van die problematiek van die diagnose van voa nie. op grond van die resultate van die huidige ondersoek kan die volgende diagnostiese kriteria van voa by die kind wat wel verbaliteit verkry het, voorgestel word: — 'n diskrepansie tussen reseptiewe taalvermoe en ekspressiewe vermoe soos onder andere weerspieel in die lengte van woorde wat geproduseer kan word; — onkonstante distorsie van konsonante en vokale; — worstel ing by of 'n on vermoe in die produksie van u itinge wat vir die kind moeilik is om te produseer; — die onvermoe tot lang uitinge lei tot fonologiese verkorting. verdere navorsing met groot groepe kinders met voa moet egter eers uitgevoer word voor hierdie kriteria as korrek aanvaar kan word. die resultate van die huidige ondersoek laat egter min twyfel oor die bestaan van voa as 'n diagnostiese entiteit. die ooreenstemmende simptome van die proefpersone asook die feit dat proefpersoon 5 op dieselfde wyse as die persone met w a gereageer het op die variasie in kontekstuele faktore is verder ook sterk aanduidings dat die onderliggende aard van verbale apraksie in die kongenitale en verworwe vorms in belangrike opsigte ooreenkom. in die literatuur word 'n beperkte siening van die aard van verbale apraksie gehandhaaf soos weerspieel in die definiering van die afwyking. die algemeen aanvaarde definisie ;van darley, aronson & brown (1975: 255) beskryf verbale apraksie as: "an articulatory disorder resulting from impairment,due to brain damage, of the capacity to program the positioning of speech musculature for the volitional production of phonemes and the sequencing of muscle movements for the production of words." | hierdie beskrywing is waarskynlik volkome korrek en kan aanvaar word, maar die totale omvang van die motoriese beplanning van spraak en moontlike afwykings daarin word nie in die definisie weerspieel nie. die beplanning van spraak kan meer omvattend beskryf word as: — die herroeping van die onveranderlike kernmotorplan met ruimtelike en temporale spesifikasies van bewegings vir elke foneem, , — die daaropvolgende aanpassing van die kernmotorplan by die klankomgewing, koartikulasiemoontlikhede en die spoed van produksie binne die grense van motoriese ekwivalensie, — die temporale organisasie van struktuurbewegings en — die sistematiese vooruitvoering in volgorde van onveranderlike fonologiese eenhede met gespesifiseerde motorplan-subroe tines, — binne die invloed van kontekstuele faktore soos die lengte, klankstruktuur, bekendheid en motoriese kompleksiteit van 'n uiting (van der merwe, 1986). binne sodanige teoretiese raamwerk kan w a gesien word as 'n afwyking in een of meer van hierdie stadiums of komponente en voa as 'n onvermoe om een of meer van hierdie stadiums of komponente te realiseer. die nie-verbale apraktiese kind leer moontlik nie kernmotorplanne vir klanke aan nie of kan die kern nie aanpas by die klankomgewing nie of kan die agtereenvolgende beplanning en produksie van klanke in 'n woord nie behartig nie. sodanige teoretiese raamwerk is vanselfsprekend slegs hipoteties, maar dit is duidelik dat logiese verklarings vir die voorkoms van 'n kongenitale en verworwe vorm van verbale apraksie daarop gebaseer kan word. indringende gevallestudies van groot groepe kinders met voa sal in die toekoms meer lig werp op die diagnostiese kenmerke en die aard van die probleem by suiwer voa. k e r n s i m p t o m e e n g e a s s o s i e e r d e s i m p t o m e in die vorige artikels (van der merwe, et al, 1987; 1988; van der merwe et al, 1989) is kernen geassosieerde simptome ge'fdentifiseer op grond van konteks-sensitiwiteit. daar is gevind dat die ouditief waarneembare simptome met die hoogste voorkoms by al die proefpersone, naamlik klankdistorsie en afwykings in temporale vloei en ook sat-foute wat dui op temporale interartikulator wankoordinasie nie gereageer het op variasie in konteks nie en dus nie konteks-sensitief was nie. daarteenoor is gevind dat simptome soos klankvervangings, veranderinge in die struktuur van die eenheid deur weglatings en byvoegings van klanke en afwykings in vokaalen uitingduur toeneem met 'n toename in die lengte of motoriese kompleksiteit van 'n uiting. hierdie simptome is dus konteks-sensitief en die afleiding is gemaak dat hierdie geassosieerde simptome moontlik kompensatoriese strategies weerspieel terwyl die simptome wat nie konteks-sensitief is nie en deurentyd in dieselfde mate voorkom waarskynlik aanduidings is van die kern van die probleem en kernsimptome genoem kan word. die huidige ondersoek se resultate toon aan dat al die proefpersone, ingeslote proefpersoon 5, oorwegend ooreenstemmend reageerop die variasie in konteks. die kernsimptome en geassosieerde simptome stem dus by almal ooreen. indien die afleiding oor die verdeling van simptome as geldig aanvaar word en die feit dat al die proefpersone oorwegend ooreenstemmende patrone van konteks-sensitiwiteit vertoon bevestig grootliks so 'n afleiding, dan is die implikasie dat die kernsimptome en geassosieerde simptome by voa en w a ooreenstem. die aard van die kernsimptome versterk verder ook die uitgangspunt dat verbale apraksie 'n afwyking in die beplanning van spraak is. gevolgtrekking — die kollektiewe voorkoms van sekere simptome lei tot die beeld van w a , maar die besondere konstellasie van simptome op grond van frekwensie van voorkoms verskil van persoon tot persoon. — klankdistorsie en/of afwykings in temporale vloei van spraak wat aanduidings is van die kern van die probleem by verbale apraksie vertoon die hoogste frekwensie van voorkoms by al die proefpersone. — die frekwensie van voorkoms van sekere simptome gee aanleiding tot individuele simptoombeelde en hierdie simptoombeelde dui op die moontlikheid van tipes verbale apraksie. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykins, vol 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 4 a . v a n d e r m e r w e & r.j. g r i m b e e k — die p r o e f p e r s o o n m e t voa ( p r o e f p e r s o o n 5) verskil i n t o t a a l die m e e s t e v a n die groep, m a a r w a t die o n d e r s k e i e a s p e k t e b e t r e f verskil hy slegs t e n o p s i g t e v a n u i t i n g d u u r e n die f r e k w e n s i e p a t r o o n v a n o u d i t i e f w a a r n e e m b a r e foute die m e e s t e v a n die groep. — p r o e f p e r s o o n 5 h e t al die s i m p t o m e g e m e e n m e t die groep e n die a a r d v a n die foute k o m o o k o o r e e n , m a a r die frekw e n s i e v a n v o o r k o m s v a n s e k e r e s i m p t o m e gee a a n l e i d ing tot ' n i n d i v i d u e l e s i m p t o o m b e e l d . — p r o e f p e r s o o n 5 is in t e e n s t e l l i n g m e t die p r o e f p e r s o n e m e t w a m e e r geneig o m die l a n g o n s i n e e n h e d e (s5) fonologies te v e r e e n v o u d i g e n v e r t o o n d a a r o m m e e r k l a n k v e r v a n g i n g s . h i e r d i e verskil k a n in s a m e h a n g m e t die totale b e e l d v a n v e r t r a a g d e s p r a a k o n t w i k k e l i n g w e e s . — h i e r d i e b e s o n d e r e p e r s o o n m e t v o a v e r t o o n m i n d e r p r o b l e m e m e t die t e m p o r a l e vloei v a n s p r a a k e n k l e i n e r afw y k i n g s i n u i t i n g d u u r as die p e r s o n e m e t w a e n d i t w i l v o o r k o m asof die s p r a a k w a a r o o r h y w e l b e s k i k m a k l i k e r vooraf b e p l a n w o r d a s w a t die geval is b y die p e r s o n e m e t w a . — h i e r d i e o n d e r s o e k o n d e r s t e u n die k o n s e p v a n v o a a s 'n d i a g n o s t i e s e e n t i t e i t e n bevestig o o k die d i a g n o s e v a n voa o p g r o n d v a n n i e v e r b a l i t e i t b y die k i n d m e t geen a n d e r b e k e n d e / o p s i g t e l i k e p r o b l e m e n i e . — die o n d e r l i g g e n d e a a r d v a n die p r o b l e e m b y v o a e n w a k o m in b e l a n g r i k e opsigte o o r e e n e n b e i d e k a n w a a r s k y n lik a s v o r m s v a n a f w y k i n g i n die b e p l a n n i n g v a n s p r a a k b e s k o u w o r d . — k o n t e k s s e n s i t i e w e s i m p t o m e e n s i m p t o m e w a t n i e kont e k s s e n s i t i e f is n i e k o m o o r e e n b y al die p r o e f p e r s o n e , i n g e s l o t e die p e r s o o n m e t voa. d i t w i l d u s v o o r k o m s asof k e r n s i m p t o m e e n g e a s s o s i e e r d e s i m p t o m e b y voa e n w a o o r e e n k o m . e r k e n n i n g s f i n a n s i e l e b y s t a n d v a n die r a a d vir g e e s t e s w e t e n s k a p l i k e n a v o r s i n g v i r h i e r d i e o n d e r s o e k , w o r d h i e r m e e e r k e n . v e r w y s i n g s aram, d.m. assessment and treatment of developmental apraxia: preface. seminars in speech and language, 5, 1984. darley, f.l., aronson, a.e. en brown ,j. motor speech disorders. philadelphia: saunders, 1975. deutsch, s.e. prediction of site of lesion from speech apraxia error patterns. in j.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil en a.e. aronson (reds.) apraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics, management. california: college-hill press, 1984. guyette, t.w. en diedrich, w.m. a critical review of developmental apraxia of speech. in ν j. lass (red.). speech and language, 5,149, 1981. itoh, m. en sasanuma, s. articulatory movements in apraxia of speech. inj.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil en a.e. aronson (reds.). apraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics, management. california: college-hill press, 1984. kent, r.d. en rosenbek, j.c. acoustic patterns of apraxia of speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 26, 231-249, 1983. kertesz, a. subcortical lesions and verbal apraxia. in j.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil en a.e. aronson (reds.). apraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics, management. california: collegehill press, 1984. macaluso-haynes, s. developmental apraxia of speech: symptoms and treatment. in d.f. johns (red.) clinical management of neurogenic communicative disorders. boston: little, brown and company, 1978. rosenbek, j.c., kent, r.d. en la pointe, l.l. aparxia of speech: an overview of some perspectives. inj.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil and a.e. aronson (reds.). apraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics, management. california: college-hill press, 1984. steyn, a.g.w., smit, g.f. en du toit, s.h.c. moderne statistiek vir die praktyk. pretoria: j.l. van schaik, 1984. van der merwe, a. terapieprogram vir verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie met moontlikhede vir ander spraakafwykings. publikasie van die universiteit van pretoria, pretoria: v&r, 1985. van der merwe, a. die motoriese beplanning van spraak by verbale apraksie. ongepubliseerde d.phil-verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria, 1986. van der merwe, α., uys, i.c., loots, j.m. en grimbeeck, r.j. die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 34, 10-22, 1987. van der merwe, α., uys, i.c., loots, j.m. en grimbeek, r.j. ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie: aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 35, 45-54, 1988. van der mewre, α., uys, i.c., loots, j.m., grimbeek, r.j. en jansen, l.p.c. die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur by verbale apraksie. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 36, 2941, 1989. wertz, r.t., la pointe, l.l. en rosenbek, j.c. apraxia of speech in adults: the disorder and its management. orlando: grune & stratton, inc., 1984. yoss, k.a en darley, f.l. developmental apraxia of speech in children with defective articulation. journal of speech and hearing research, 17, 399-416, 1974. | the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a phonological analysis of the expressive and receptive articulatory difficulties of an aphasic with apraxia of speech: a case study. aura kagan, b.a. (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand). dept. speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary the expressive and receptive phonological errors of an aphasic subject with mild apraxia o f s p e e c h were analysed in terms of a distinctive feature framework. the results indicated that errors could be characterized linguistically and t h a t such information could be of therapeutic significance. the relationship between articulation problems and ability to discriminate phonemes was investigated. although no direct relationship was found, discrimination errors followed linguistic trends demonstrated in t h e articulation errors. the findings of this study suggest that t h e traditional idea of apraxia as a non-linguistic and purely m o t o r disorder needs re-examination. opsomming die ekspressiewe en reseptiewe fonologiese foute van 'n afatiese proefpersoon met matige spraakapraksie is ontleed in 'n raamwerk van distinktiewe eienskappe. die uitslae dui aan dat foute linguisties gekaraktiseer kan word en dat die inligting dan van terapeutiese waaide k o n wees. die verwantskap tussen artikulasieprobleme en die vermoe om tussen foneme te diskrimineer is ondersoek. alhoewel daar geen direkte verwantskap gevind is nie, het diskriminasiefoute dieselfde linguistiese neigings getoon as die artikulasiefoute. die bevindings van hierdie studie dui aan dat die tradisionele begtip van apraksie as 'n nie-linguistiese en suiwer motoriese afwyking hersien behoort te woord. apraxia of speech has been considered to be a nonlinguistic disorder.27 for the purpose of this study, the term apraxia will be used as defined by de renzi et al,7 who refer to oral apraxia as . . the inability to perform voluntary movements with the muscles of the larynx, pharynx, tongue, lips and cheeks, although automatic movements of the same muscles are preserved. the possibility of the application of phonological theory to apraxia occurring within the aphasic syndrome is felt to be of interest, as much recent work in the field of aphasia has indicated that articulation errors can be systematically described and analysed in terms of phonological theory. • • • > it must be pointed out that there has been controversy as to the relevance of phonological theory in the field of aphasia. critchley,4 for example, concludes that: to trace any rigid plan underlying the pattern of articulatory disorders in aphasic patients is premature. spreen,3 3 however, points out that a phonological investigation of aphasia is valuable, as phonological errors, besides being of inherent linguistic interest, tend to confuse the study of higher-level speech functions. the writer was interested in undertaking a phonological analysis of apraxic errors based on articulation trends derived from recent work in the field of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 aura kagan aphasia and phonology. some of this work has taken place within the framework of distinctive feature (df) theory.2' 2 1 ' 2 2 this theory, first proposed by jakobson et a l , 1 3 and later developed by chomsky and halle,3 has at its foundation the belief that the distinctive features making up the phoneme are the basic units of language. standel et a l 3 4 define distinctive features as . . . the smallest individual characteristic of a particular phoneme that can determine a difference between phonemes. blumstein,2 as well as martin and rigrodsky,23 found that the frequency of substitution errors made by aphasic subjects was inversely related to df distance. results of the latter study also revealed that errors on df oppositions formed a definite hierarchy. blumstein 2 also found that unmarked phonemes were substituted for marked phonemes more than vice-versa. the concept of 'markedness' in,phono logical theory is complicated and is defined differently within different theoretical frameworks. blumstein 2 introduces the idea of a hierarchical relationship between phonemes, the marked value being more complex than the unmarked or more basic value. she gives the example of the relationship between / ρ / and i b / where / ρ / is the unmarked member to which the feature (+ voice) is added in order to obtain / b / which is thus marked in relation to / ρ /. another interesting feature of the application of phonological theory to aphasia arises from jakobson's notions of the inverse relationship between childhood acquisition and aphasic dissolution of the phonological system.12 it was felt that it would be of value to investigate whether apraxic errors occurring within the aphasic syndrome could be analysed in terms of this theory. the writer was also interested in an analysis of the self-correction of articulation, as this presents us with an opportunity of observing phonological processing in action. in addition, self-correction embodies auditory discrimination, 5 ' 6 and thus the relationship between this analysis and the results of discrimination testing should be of interest. it is felt that the application of phonological theory to apraxia could further our knowledge of this disorder, as the usefulness of conventional tests of articulation appears to be limited, particularly in cases where the apraxic element is of a milder or more subtle nature. the rationale for this study was thus to investigate whether a phonological approach would yield more meaningful results than conventional articulation tests of production and discrimination in the description and analysis of apraxic articulatory errors. the writer felt that if this did prove to be the case, such an approach could be effectively used in the drawing-up of therapy programmes for these patients. / / method s u b j e c t (s) the s used in this study was an adult white male aged 45 years. severe aphasia involving apraxia developed as the result of the removal of infected braintissue. the infection set in after surgery to treat a subdural haemorrhage caused by trauma to the left cerebral hemisphere. the s had been receiving speech therapy for two years and had improved greatly during this time. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological analysis of an aphasic with apraxia 25 the s selected fulfilled the following criteria: 1) he was diagnosed as aphasic by a speech therapist and a neurologist. this was further confirmed by a formal rating on the boston diagnostic test of aphasia.10 2) apraxic difficulties of a mild nature were experienced. a mild case was chosen as it was hoped that this would serve to highlight the value of a linguistic approach as compared to conventional testing. 3) dysarthria was ruled out as being etiologically related to the articulation problem. 4) the s had an adequate pre-morbid acquisition of speech and language. 5) hearing was within normal limits. a i m s 1) to investigate whether or not a df analysis would be more successful than conventional tests in the description and analysis of apraxic articulatory errors. 2) to observe whether linguistic trends emerge in apraxic substitution errors as tested on a nonsense-syllable repetition task. (see [4] in section on tests employed for the rationale behind the use of nonsense syllables.) in particular, to investigate: a) whether there is an inverse relationship between the number of errors and df distance between the target phoneme and substitution error; b) whether or not certain feature oppositions emerge as being more diffi. cult than others; c) if unmarked phonemes are substituted for marked phonemes more than vice-versa; d) whether certain phonemes are substituted more often than others, regardless of the particular target phoneme; e) whether attempts at self-correction move towards the target phoneme or if they are random. 3) to test discrimination of phonemes in nonsense syllables on a pointing task, in terms of findings on the nonsense-syllable repetition task. 4) to assess the value of a linguistic approach for therapy. t e s t s e m p l o y e d i n t h e s t u d y (construction, administrative procedures and scoring) 1. the boston diagnostic aphasia examination 1 0 in addition to obtaining a formal confirmation of the clinical diagnosis of aphasia and assessing the degree of articulatory difficulty, this test was used in order to obtain a profile of scores on various linguistic dimensions. administration followed the procedures laid down in the test manual. 2. the goldman-fristoe test of articulation.9 this was administered twice: a) conventionally, i.e. the s was required to name pictured objects and describe pictured situations, and b) the s had to repeat the names of objects or descriptions of situations after the experimendie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 aura kagan ter, (e). (see 2 in results and discussion for the rationale behind the two administrations of this test). 3. spontaneous speech sample forty minutes of speech was elicited by requesting descriptions of pictures such as those from the boston diagnostic aphasia examination.10 the sample was recorded on a sony tape recorder (tc 350). 4. nonsense syllable repetition task nonsense syllables were included in the test battery because, as pointed out by martin and rigrodsky,21 they might be more representative of a 'purely phonological task' in that they exclude semantic factors. in addition, nonsense syllables have the advantage of being able to be systematically manipulated, e.g. each phoneme can be systematically tested in different positions and in the context of other phonemes. in order to facilitate the drawing up of the nonsense syllables as well as subsequent analysis, computer programmes were constructed based on the df chart proposed by chomsky and halle.3 this theoretical framework was chosen as it is considered to be one of the most comprehensive to date. 1 1 only the consonantal system was investigated, which means that the number of relevant distinctive features was limited to eight of the original thirteen proposed, viz: (± voice), (± continuant), (± nasal), (± strident), (± high), (± back), (+ anterior) and (± coronal). based on this, the following programmes were devised:a). all possible consonant contrasts grouped according to the df distance between them, with a list of the features on which the differences occurred. b). contrasts grouped in terms of features (e.g. all contrasts involving (± voice) in a hierarchy ranging from a difference of one distinctive feature upwards.) c). contrasts grouped in terms of particular phonemes (e.g. all contrasts involving / ρ /). d). contrasts grouped in terms of the order of differences (e.g. all contrasts involving (± coronal);(± coronal) + (± voice);(± coronal) + (± voice) + (± continuant). the nonsense syllables were constructed as follows: i. they consisted of 3 phonemes in cvc combinations. ii. initial and final consonants differed by at least three distinctive features (although in certain cases, contrasts of two distinctive features apart had to be used in order to avoid the formation of meaningful words). this was based on a finding by lecours and lhermitte^18 that the likelihood of phonological error on , a particular phoneme is directly related to the extent of its similarity to other phonemes in its immediate vicinity. iii. each phoneme was tested in the context of two vowels and two consonants (voiced-voiceless cognates). the feature (± voice) was chosen because it is felt that its positive and negative values represented a clear acoustic and articulatory distinction. specific vowels and consonants were chosen on the basis of avoiding the formation of meaningful words. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological analysis of an aphasic with apraxia 27 it was hoped by the above procedure to control for the influence of contextual environment on the test-phoneme to some minimal extent. iv. each phoneme was tested in initial and final position. where possible, the consonants were merely reversed, keeping the vowel constant, in order that only one variable be manipulated at a time, viz: position. v. where possible, phonemes were tested in blends (in initial position only). the blends were those used by johnson, darley and spriestersb a c h 1 6 in their articulation test form, viz: /1 /, / s / and / r / . (see table i for an example of nonsense syllable manipulation.) initial position final position. blends /kov/; /kif/ /vok/; /fik/ /krov/; /kriv/ /klov/; /kliv/ /skov/; /skiv/ table i: example of the testing of the phoneme / k / . procedure for the testing of nonsense syllables: a) the entire nonsense syllable test was administered twice: (i) in the early morning and (ii) in the late afternoon, each administration involving several sessions. both the s and the ε felt that testing in this manner sampled performance at its best and at its worst. apraxia is, by its very definition, inconsistent.5' 6 it seems logical therefore, to include the inconsistencies as part of the data. it is felt that many studies in the field of apraxia can be criticised for not repeating test procedures more than o n c e . 6 ' 1 5 b) the s was required to repeat the nonsense syllables spoken by the ε. he was seated at right angles to the tester in such a manner as to obtain only auditory stimuli, as according to johns and darley,1 5 articulation proficiency is aided by the use of both auditory and visual modes of stimulation. c) all substitution errors were noted. the decision to use this type of error was based on findings of recent research,21 where it has been found that substitutions (or commutation errors) occurred most frequently in nonsense-syllable repetition tasks. d) the s's responses were noted by both the ε and an observer. wherever the two versions did not agree, the error was not included in the analysis. a tape-recorder was not used as this had been found to adversely affect the s's performance. for this same reason, the observer was a person familiar to the s. s c o r i n g a) responses were recorded phonologically and the results were tabled so as to give information about the df distance between the target phoneme die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 77 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 aura kagan and substitution, as well as an indication of what these differences were. (see table ii.) a particular substitution had to occur at least twice before it was considered to be an error. a separate analysis of phoneme contexts and positions was felt to be beyond the scope of this study. these contexts and positions were included so that the phonemes could be tested under varied conditions. in this way, the results could not be attributed to variables external to the phoneme itself. target phoneme substitution v, cont. n, s . £l b. a. c. total ρ ρ m t * * * 2 1 total 1 1 1 key: v = voice, cont. = continuant, ν = nasal, s = strident, η = high, β = back, a = anterior, c = coronal table ii: example of the df distance between target phoneme and substitution, with the features on which the phonemes differ. in relation to table ii, a horizontal score tally gives the total df distance between the target phoneme arid substitution while a vertical score tally gives the number of times errors occurred on particular feature oppositions. b) the latter results were ranked and spearman's rank-order correlation test was performed between this result and that obtained by martin and rigrodsky 2 2 on a similar task. target phoneme 1st attempt 2nd attempt 3rd attempt ρ s f t phoneme contrasts df distance ρ s 3ρ f ? / ρ t 1 / in this example, attempts at self-correction moved towards the target phoneme in terms of df distance. table iii: example of the analysis of self-correction attempts. c) substitution errors were analysed in order to see whether voiceless and anthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological analysis of an aphasic with apraxia 29 terior phonemes were used as substitutions more often than voiced and back phonemes. d) substitution errors were compared with the target phoneme in terms of whether unmarked features were substituted for marked features more than vice versa. analyses were done on three different sets of data: i) phoneme contrasts involving a df distance of 1, after blumstein's procedure.2 ii) phoneme contrasts involving a df distance of 1 but excluding all those involving the phoneme / ρ / . xn examination of the df chart of chomsky and halle,3 reveals that / ρ / is the only phoneme with one positive distinctive feature (excluding the feature [± consonantal] which is common to all phonemes used in this study). therefore, it is theoretically impossible for / ρ / to be substituted for by an unmarked phoneme; as such a phoneme would have no positive distinctive features. it was therefore felt that the inclusion of / ρ / may bias the results. iii) all phoneme contrasts. for this purpose, the marked phoneme of the pair was defined as the one with more positive distinctive features. both (ii) and (iii) represent extensions of the procedure used by blumstein.2 e) attempts at self-correction were analysed in terms of whether they moved towards the target phoneme or not. (see table iii.) general points regarding procedure: testing took place over approximately fourteen hours, involving thirty-eight sessions. testing sessions were short, the exact time involved depending on the s's reaction. the experimental procedure was made as flexible as was practical in order to elicit maximal performance. the test atmosphere was relaxed and informal so that variables such as test anxiety would not contaminate the results. this approach to testing has been supported by authorities such as schuell et a l . 3 0 although discrimination testing formed a large part of this study, it is not within the scope of this paper to discuss it in depth. a brief summary of procedures and results of discrimination testing can be found at the end of the following section. results and discussion 1. the boston diagnostic aphasia examination the s received a severity rating of 2, which, according to goodglass and kaplan, ,10 indicates that there are . . . frequent failures to convey the idea, but patient shares the burden of communication with the examiner. although it was difficult to give the profile a specific label as the scores did not fall into an easily recognisable stereotype, the use of a score profile is felt to be a great advantage of this test as it enables one to see the relationship between various areas of deficit. it is felt, however, that the test is lacking in the depth required die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 aura kagan for planning detailed therapy procedures. for example, the writer found it difficult to obtain an in-depth picture of the articulatory difficulties of the s. 2. the goldman-fristoe test of articulation neither this test nor the spontaneous speech sample are discussed in detail as they do not form the focus of the study, but were used to reveal the fact that conventional testing procedures were not always suitable for ascertaining the extent of the subtle articulation difficulty being experienced by the s. errors made in response to the conventional administration of the goldman-fristoe test9 were felt to berelated to naming difficulties rather than to articulation problems. for example, when shown a picture of a 'house', the s responded with: "building no, to live as usual i can't remember box . . ." when the test was modified in such a way that the s repeated the name after the e, he managed successfully in almost every case. 3. spontaneous speech sample surprisingly, only 5 substitution errors were noted in this forty-minute speech sample. possible explanations relate to the help afforded by semantic content, pictures which did not require much interpretative skill, and the relaxed, informal atmosphere of the test situation. as well as this, the s had the choice of production under his control. based on the results of the above two tests, it might have been decided that the s did not have phonological problems. however, the clinical observations that the s's articulatory performance did in fact break down under difficult conditions (e.g. when tired, upset, nervous, speaking to strangers or describing specific situations), indicated that more sensitive testing was required. 4. nonsense syllable repetition task (a) the relationship between number of errors and df distance. as seen in figure 1, there is an inverse trend in the relationship between error frequency 12 10 error 8 frequency 6 4 2 / 1 2 3 4 5 df distance figure 1: relationship between error frequency and df distance between target phoneme and substitution error. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological analysis of an aphasic with apraxia 31 and df distance, confirming the results of the studies of blumstein 2 and martin and rigrodsky. 2 2 most errors occurred on a df distance of 1 and least on 5. according to blumstein,2an essential notion in df theory is that phonemes with a df distance of 1 are more similar . . . structurally, motorically and acoustically, as well as psychologically . .. than phonemes with a larger df difference. lecours and lhermitte18 in their measures of paradigmatic distance, based on five parameters, found that most substitution errors occurred between morphologically similar phonemic units. although it is difficult to compare the results of studies based on different theoretical frameworks, it is interesting to note the similarity of general tendencies. this leads the writer to believe that we are dealing with psychologically valid phenomena and not merely results determined by particular theories. although most errors did occur between similar phonemes in terms of the df framework of chomsky and halle, 3 this theory could not explain certain substitution errors with a larger df distance. some of these are better explained by the jakobsonian framework. 1 1 ' 1 3 it seems, for example, that in certain instances acoustic dimensions do function as superior explanatory tools, e.g. with / g / / b / ; / k / / p / ; / r ) / / m / , all have a df distance of 3 in terms of chomsky and halle's theory,3 but in terms of the jakobsonian df framework,11 labials and velars share the property of ( + grave) or 'low tonality', and are thus close in acoustic terms. the finding that substitution errors are not random, but are in most cases similar to the stimulus, leads the writer to feel, in common with martin,20 that apraxia of speech within the aphasic syndrome is a linguistic disorder and should as such be considered as an integral part of the aphasic impairment. this finding has important implications for therapy and suggests that the difficulties might be amenable to df therapy. (b) hierarchy of feature opposition difficulty: table iv indicates that certain features emerge as being more difficult than others. these results were correlated with those found by martin and rigrodsky. 2 2 the correlation was not significant ( r s = 0.38 ρ > 0.05). this serves as a reminder that care must be exercised before generalizing from the results of feature no. of errors rank coronal 11 1 anterior 7 2 back voice high 3 strident nasal continuant 5 4 3 1 4 5 6 7 table iv: hierarchy of difficulty on feature oppositions die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 aura kagan research to the individual patient. general trends are useful as an indication of what to expect, based on the average scores of many ss. they cannot predict with complete accuracy, the performance of any one individual. although the writer recognises the inconsistency of apraxic articulation errors, the fact that the phonemes were tested in different contexts at different times, makes it possible to draw tentative conclusions relevant to the planning of therapy for this patient. table iv presents a hierarchy of difficulty through which to work. the writer feels that these features themselves can be systematically analysed to give specific starting points for therapy. this point is discussed in detail at a later stage in this paper. (c) analysis of substitution errors: according to jakobson,12 phonological disintegration in the aphasic is an 'exact mirror-image' of phonological acquisition in the child. other authorities, such as critchley 8 disagree with this point of view. in order to investigate jakobson's hypothesis, one should select trends which have been well established as characteristic of the childhood acquisition of sounds. hyman1 1 feels that certain of the trends observed by jakobson are reliable, although the details have not always been supported by recent studies. he gives as examples, the acquisition of voiceless before voiced stops, as well as the acquisition of front before back consonants. if jakobson's theory 1 2 is valid, one would expect the aphasic to find voiceless and anterior phonemes easier than voiced and back phonemes, and would therefore predict that they would be used more frequently as substitutions. the results obtained indicate that voiceless stops were substituted more than their voiced cognates in every case (figure 2) and anterior phonemes were used as substitutions more frequently than back phonemes (figure 3). figure 2 also indicates that this trend applied to voiced-voiceless cognates other than stops. it is interesting to note that within the markedness theory of generative phonology, voiceless stops are 'universally less marked' than their voiced cognates. 1 1 18 16 frequency 14 of 12 occurrence 10 of 8 / errors 6 4 2 i « 1 » « 1 « « p-b t-d f-v k-g s-z c-j θ-8 voiced voiceless cognates figure 2: voiced compared to voiceless substitution errors the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological analysis of an aphasic with apraxia 33 mean 8 frequency 6 of 4 occurrence 2 anterior* back** •anterior was defined as any phoneme with the feature ( + anterior) **back was defined as any phoneme with the feature ( — anterior) (both based on chomsky & halle's df chart3). figure 3: anterior ̂ compared to back substitution errors. (d) markedness analysis: figure 4 represents the trend for unmarked phonemes to be substituted for marked phonemes more than vice-versa, using three different manipulations of the substitution errors made by the s. theoretically these results are significant, as they lend credence to the idea put forward by chomsky and halle3 t h a t . . . unmarked features do not add to the complexity of a grammar. the writer agrees with blumstein2 that such results can be seen within the general trend in aphasia towards... simplification of the phonological system. contrasts with contrasts with contrasts df distance df distance involving df of 1 of 1 excluding distance of contrasts l and more involving /p/ key: v / x \ .'marked to unmarked i i : unmarked to marked figure 4: markedness analysis a comparison of substitution error to target phoneme die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 aura kagan 80 % 60 error 40 20 towards target phoneme away from target phoneme figure 5: analysis of self-correction errors (e) analysis of the process of spontaneous self-correction: the results in figure 5 indicate that in the majority of cases, attempts at selfcorrection move towards the target phoneme in terms of df distance. this is an interesting result as it appears to tap an ongoing phonological process rather than examining a static structure. the writer feels that these results indicate the applicability of distinctive features, which are an abstract concept, to a psychologically real process. in his efforts to reach the target, the s did appear to be using distinctive features. this is felt to be a striking example of the fact that the apraxic errors in this aphasic s could be linguistically characterised, and implies that linguistics, or more specifically, phonological theory, could be of use in planning therapy for him. the concept of a feature opposition chart as an aid to df therapy before discussing this procedure, it is felt that the use of df therapy in aphasia needs justification. much recent work in the field of articulation difficulties in children has been involved with an application of phonological theory. 2 4 ' 2 s > 2 6 most studies seem to be based on jakobson's idea that childhood acquisition of phonology can be seen in terms of the acquisition of feature contrasts. if one accepts jakobson's theory12, about the relationship between aphasia and childhood language, it is possible that the dissolution of the phonological system in aphasia could be viewed in terms of the loss of feature contrasts. the results of this study show that the apraxic errors of an aphasic patient can be usefully described and analysed within a df framework. it seems logical, therefore, to attempt to remediate within the same framework. it must be pointed out that there have been criticisms of this approach. walsh,35 for example, feels that distinctive features are too abstract to be clinically useful. based on the finding that most of this s's errors occurred on the feature (± coronal), a coronal feature opposition chart was devised as an aid to therapy. the chart is a representation of all possible contrasts involving (± coronal) in terms of chomsky and halle's theory.3 , the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological analysis of an aphasic with apraxia 35 df distance c 1 p t ; b d ; f s ; v z ; m n . . 2 c + v 2 p d ; b—t; f-z; v s . 3 cont. ν str. η β a 3 ρ i ρ 1 b θ ρ η t m f 3 f 1 ν θ 4 ν str a cont cont β a a str cont str η 4 θ m p z b s t v d f p r θ m p z b s t v d f c g j k f r f 5 v-j j k i-t p r f r f 3 v j key: c=coronal v v o i c e cont. =continuant n = nasal str. = strident h=high β = back a = anterior table v: example of part of the coronal feature opposition chart. similar charts could be drawn up for any feature. the chart can be used to describe df distance as well as to see the effects of particular feature combinations. as previously noted, most substitution errors occurred between phonemes with a df distance of 1 and least between phonemes with a df distance of 5. therefore, when beginning distinctive-feature therapy, one would initially choose contrasts with a large df difference, gradually and systematically decreasing this until the patient can contrast differences of 1 distinctive feature. winitz 3 6 mentions a similar principle with reference to 'phonetic' distance. if df training is to begin with phonemes having a df distance of more than 1, it becomes necessary to analyse the effects of various features in combination with the coronal feature. for example, if at a df level of 2, the s makes least errors when the coronal feature is combined with voice, therapy could begin with this combination. looking at the chart for coronal plus voice (see table v), one can systematically select particular phoneme contrasts for inclusion in the df therapy programme. in general, one would work from greatest to smallest df distance. in addition, within eachdf level, one should work within a hierarchy of difficulty as presented by each individual case. an example of a hierarchy of difficulty, working from the greatest to the smallest df difference, would be the following: coronal, voice, continuant, strident, e.g. p z coronal, voice, continuant, e.g. p 1 coronal, voice, e.g. p d coronal, e.g. p—t die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 aura kagan a feature opposition chart can be used to represent both errors of articulation and discrimination. by marking the errors on the chart, one is provided with an instant visual indication of feature confusions. summary of the procedures and results of discrimination testing discrimination was first tested conventionally on a modified version of the d.ip. test. 3 2 modifications were made to those americanisms felt to be confusing for the s. the s made only 4 errors but as in the case of the repetition task, it was felt that deeper testing, using a phonological approach, would reveal the fact that difficulty was being experienced in this area. therefore, a discrimination task was devised which involved pointing to one of two visually presented nonsense syllables, following an auditory stimulus given by the e. nonsense syllables were devised in the same manner as that described in the repetition task. each contrast was tested twice and the order of presentation in terms of spatial cues within the contrasts was randomized. the two stimuli were identified by the ε before the contrast was tested so that errors could not be attributed to problems of visual recognition. the following discrimination tests were administered:a) the discrimination of phoneme contrasts with a df distance of 1 which were confused on the repetition task was compared to the discrimination of phoneme contrasts having an identical df distance not derived from articulation errors. the df distance of 1 was chosen because most errors occurred here. b) in terms of findings on the nonsense-syllable repetition task, the discrimination of all possible contrasts with a df distance of 1 was compared with the discrimination of all possible contrasts involving adf distance of 5. results for a) and b) were scored in terms of i) correct/incorrect from which a percentage of error was derived, and ii) reaction time (rt), defined as the time taken between the auditory stimulus being given by the ε and.the s's pointing to one of the nonsense syllables, irrespective of whether it was correct or incorrect. it was hypothesized that rt would be a function of the 'difficulty' or 'complexity' of the stimulus. rt was measured with a stopwatch and was calculated to the nearest 1/loth second. the mean (x), standard deviation (s.d.) and range of each set of rt scores was computed, the range being defined as χ — 1 s . d . 2 8 stimuli % error χ rt sd y range substitution error — target phoneme 40 1,82 ,33 1 , 4 9 2 , 1 5 other contrasts 29 1,75 ,33 1 , 4 2 2 , 0 8 table vi: a comparison between phoneme pairs derived from substitution errors with pairs not derived from substitution errors (all having a df distance of 1) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological analysis of an aphasic with apraxia 37 the results in table vi indicate that the s did find the discrimination of his errors slightly more difficult as indicated by a larger percentage error score and longer rt. however, this difference is not considered to be reliable, as the range of scores in both cases overlapped considerably.28 the difference between discrimination of phoneme pairs with a df distance of 1 compared to those with a df distance of 5 (table vii) appears to be reliable, as indicated by a larger difference in the percentage error score and rt as well as by the fact that the range of scores hardly overlapped.28 df distance «error χ rt sd range 1 26 1,22 ,24 0 , 9 8 1 , 4 6 5 3 1,81 ,40 1 , 4 1 2 , 2 1 table vii: a comparison between the discrimination of all phoneme pairs having a df distance of 1 with all phonemes having a df distance of 5 these two findings seem to be indicative of different aspects of discrimination. the results in table vi are felt to reflect the lack of a direct relationship between articulation and discrimination in this s. this is surprising in terms of the views put forward by authorities such as lieberman19 and ladefoged et a l , 1 7 who stress the direct relationship between articulation and auditory perception (although their theories tend to emphasize different aspects of this). the results in table vii are felt to indicate the existence of an indirect relationship between articulation and discrimination in the s. as can be seen, the trend for error frequency to be inversely related to df distance followed that found in the analysis of articulatory substitution errors, i.e. phoneme contrasts with a df distance of 1 resulted in more errors and a longer rt than phonemes with adf distance of 5. . conclusions articulation errors made by the s were able to be described and analysed within a linguistic framework which provides support for the view put forward by martin 2 0 that apraxia of speech is a linguistic disorder. this implies that apraxia could be considered an integral part of the aphasic breakdown, necessitating a linguistic approach to testing and therapy. johns and darley,15 on the other hand, feel that apraxia of speech requires a different therapeutic approach to that used in aphasia. the results of the df analysis on the nonsense syllable task appear to approximate the real-life performance of this s better than the results of conventional tests which were unable to tap the subtle phonological difficulties being experienced. the s did demonstrate problems of auditory discrimination, but these were not directly related to the articulation errors, according to the results of this die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 aura kagan study. the writer feels that, as yet, we do not have enough information to make emphatic statements about the lack of a one-to-one relationship between articulation and discrimination in apraxia. in order to do so, one should test discrimination of an error at the instant that it is produced. testing the discrimination of errors at a later stage has an inherent limitation in view of the inconsistent nature of apraxic errors. it does, however, seem that there is some involvement of auditory perceptual difficulty in this s, which should be taken into account when planning therapy. the writer agrees with the views of martin et a l 2 3 who feel that aphasia is characterized by an impairment of the interaction of several processes rather than impairment of an isolated aspect of functioning. although the use of feature opposition charts involves a great deal of work on the part of the clinician, its great advantage lies in the fact that it allows the patient to demonstrate his idiosyncratic phonological difficulties in great detail, which should lead to the planning of more effective therapy. further research may serve to refine these charts in order to make them more clinically practical. it should be pointed out that the proposed approach in this study need not be limited to apraxia of speech, but should prove useful in many cases involving multiple articulation disorders. references 1. aten, j. l„ johns, d. f. & darley, f. l. (1971): auditory perception of sequenced words in apraxia of speech. j. speech hear. res., 14(1) 131-143. 2. blumstein, s. (1973): some phonological implications of aphasic speech. in psycholinguistics and aphasia, goodglass, h. & blumstein, s. (eds.) the johns hopkins university press, baltimore. 3. chomsky, n. & halle, m. (1968): the sound pattern of english. harper & row, new york. 4. critchley, m. (1973): articulatory defects in aphasia: the problem of broca's aphemia. in psycholinguistics and aphasia, goodglass h. & blumstein, s. (eds.) the johns hopkins university press, baltimore. 5. darley, f. l. (1968): apraxia of speech: 107 years of terminological confusion. paper presented to the american speech and hearing association convention. / 6. deal, j. l. & darley, f. l. (1972): the influence of linguistic and situational variables on phonemic accuracy in apraxia of speech. j. speech hear. res., 15(3), 639-653. 7. de renzi, pieczuro, a. & vignolo, l. a. (1966): oral apraxia and aphasia. cortex, 2, 50-70. 8. fry, d. b. (1959): phonemic substitutions in an aphasic patient. lang. speech, 2, 52-61. 9. goldman, r. & fristoe, m. (1969): goldman-fristoe test of articulation. american guidance service, inc., minnesota. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological analysis of an aphasic with apraxia 39 10. goodglass, h. & kaplan, e.'(1972): the assessment of aphasia and related disorders. lea & febeger, philadelphia. 11. hyman, l. m. (1975): phonology: theory and analysis. holt, rinehart & winston, u. s. a. 12. jakobson, r. (1968): child language, aphasia and phonological universals. mouton, the hague. 13. jakobson, r., fant, g. & halle, m. (1961): preliminaries to speech analysis. m.i.t. press. cambridge, massachusetts. 14. johns, d. f. (1970): application of experimental evidence in the treatment of apraxia of speech. paper presented to the american speech hearing association convention. 15. johns, d. f. & darley, f. l. (1970): phonemic variability in apraxia of speech./. speech hear. res., 13(3), 556-583. 16. johnson, w„ darley, f. l. & spriestersbach, d. c. (1963): diagnostic methods in speech pathology. harper & row, new york. 17. ladefoged, p., de clerk, j., lindau, m. & papcun, g. (1972): an auditory-motor theory of speech production. unpublished paper, u.c.l.a. working papers in phonetics, 22, 48-75. 18. lecours, a. r. & lhermitte, f. (1973): phonemic paraphasias: linguistic structures and tentative hypotheses. in psycholinguistics and aphasia, goodglass, h. & blumstein, s. (eds.). the johns hopkins university press, baltimore. 19. lieberman, p. (1972): speech acoustics and perception. the bobbsmerrill company, inc., u. s. a. 20. martin, a. d. (1974): some objections to the term apraxia of speech. j. speech hear. dis., 39(1), 54-63. 21. martin, a. d. & rigrodsky, s. (1974a): an investigation of phonological impairment in aphasia. part i. cortex, 10(4), 317-328. 22. martin, a. d. & rigrodsky, s. (1974 b): an investigation of phonological impairment in aphasia. part ii. cortex, 10(4), 329-346. 23. martin, a. d„ wasserman, ν. h„ gilden, l„ gerstman, l. & west, j. (1975): a process model of repetition in aphasia: an investigation of phonological and morphological interactions in aphasic error performance. brain language, 2(4), 434-450. 24. mcreynolds, l. v. & engmann, d. l. (1975): distinctive feature analysis of misarticulations. university park press, baltimore. 25. mcreynolds, l. v. & huston, k. (1971): a distinctive feature analysis of children's misarticulations. j. speech hear. dis., 36(2), 155166. 26. menyuk, p. (1968): the role of distinctive features in children's acquisition of phonology.speech hear. res., 11(1), 138-146. 27. rosenbek, j. c„ lemme, m. l„ ahem, μ. b„ harris, ε. h. & wertz, r. t. (1973): a treatment for apraxia of speech in adults. j. speech hear. dis., 38(4), 462-472. 28. saling, m. (1976): lecturer, department of psychology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, personal communication. 29. schuell, h. (1966): some dimensions of aphasic impairment in adults considered in relationship to investigation of language disturbance in children. brit. j. dis. commun., 1(1), 33-45. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 aura kagan 30. schuell, h„ jenkins, j. j. & jiminez-pab'on,(1964): aphasia in adults. harper & row, new york. 31. sankweiler,d.&harris,k: s.(1973): some phonological implications of aphasic speech. in psycholinguistics and aphasia, goodglass, h. & blumstein, s. (eds.). the johns hopkins university press, baltimore. 32. siegenthaler, β. m. & haspiel, g. s. (1968): discrimination by identification of pictures. pennsylvania state university speech and hearing clinic. 33. spreen, o. (1968): psycholinguistic aspects of aphasia./. speech hear. res., 11(3), 467-480. 34. standel, j. j., gardner, j. o. & hannah, e. p. (1974): distinctive feature analysis. in applied linguistic analysis, hannah e. p. (ed.). joyce publications, california. 35. walsh, h. (1974): on certain practical inadequacies of distinctive feature systems. / . speech hear. dis., 39(1), 32-43. 36. winitz, h. (1975): from syllable to conversation. university park press, baltimore. b o o k s o n speed ι and hearing c o n s u l t campus bookshop 34 bertha street, p.o. box 31361 braamfontein 2017 telephone 39-1711 westdene medio books 35/6 nedbank plaza, 175 beatrix street pretoria telephone 2-6336 logans university bookshop . . 227/9 francois road, durban telephone 35-4111 overseas publications obtained promptly. nationwide mail-order service. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) modern earing aid (pty) ltd for hearing aids — a comprehensive range comprising the latest microson directional models — unl· tron high level compression models. ear moulds — we specialise in ear mould technology to the great benefit of our patients — we also supply instant mould kits to schools. audiometers — audiotone portable screening and diagnostic units at the best prices available. sound proof booths and earmuffs high quality locally manufactured products tv accessories — manufactured by ourselves w e a r e a t 305 rand centra! 185 jeppe st johannesburg tel: 37-1774 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the iconicity of picture communications symbols for rural zulu children 40 the iconicity of picture communication symbols for rural zulu children lize haupt and erna alant* centre for augmentative and alternative communication university of pretoria abstract the purpose of this study was to investigate the iconicity of selected picture communication symbols (pcs) for rural zulu ten-year-olds. participants were presented with copies of a commercially available communication overlay without glosses. they were required to match a symbol with each of 36 spoken zulu labels. with both strict and lenient scoring criteria applied, 2.8% and 11.1% (respectively) of the symbols on the communication overlay emerged as iconic for participants. it was further established that the position of symbols on the overlay, the total frequency of selection of symbols, and gender did not influence results. an analysis of errors revealed that for some symbols many of the participants agreed on a single specific label, be it the target label or a non-target label; while for other symbols there were either many possible labels, or none. the term distinctiveness was coined to describe how well defined or specific were the evoked meanings triggered by a symbol in the viewers' minds. results suggest that participants did not make maximum use of the information provided by arrows in the symbols. this finding could be ascribed to the opaqueness of arrows and participants' lack of previous experience with these conventional cues in pictures, as well as the traditional oral nature of the zulu culture. key words: augmentative and alternative communication (aac), communication overlay, cross-cultural, iconicity, zulu, picture communication symbols (pcs), translation introduction it is often stated that good practice when first teaching symbols, is to select symbols that are easy to learn (fuller, 1997; lloyd & fuller, 1990; mirenda & locke, 1989). this strategy facilitates communication while at the same time ensuring success, which motivates the user. iconicity information can greatly aid clinicians in such a selection, since iconic symbols are easier to learn (fuller, 1987; fuller, 1997; lloyd & fuller, 1990; lloyd, fuller & arvidson, 1997; lloyd, loeding & doherty, 1985; luftig, 1983; luftig, page & lloyd, 1983; mizuko, 1987). furthermore, information about the iconicity of symbols is especially valuable in south africa because of widespread illiteracy. a literate communication partner can read the gloss (written text) that accompanies a symbol, but illiterate partners have to rely on the iconicity of symbols to guess their meaning. it is expensive and virtually impossible to train all possible communication partners in the use of the relevant symbol set/system, therefore the use of iconic symbols is more efficient. iconicity is defined as the degree to which an individual perceives visual similarity between a symbol and its referent (blischak, lloyd & fuller, 1997). factors influencing visual perception would probably influence a symbol's iconicity for a given viewer. such factors include the material on which symbols are printed (deregowski, 1980a,b); schooling (duncan, gourlay & hudson, 1973); thinking styles (retief, 1988; taylor & clarke, 1994; witkin, 1967); oral or literate background of viewer (ong, 1982) and previous experience with symbols (deloache, 1991). closer inspection of these factors reveals that they are all intertwined with culture. culture is 'a set of behaviours, institutions, beliefs, technologies and values invented and passed on by a group of individuals to sustain what they believe to be a high quality of life and to negotiate their environments' (taylor & clarke, 1994, p.-103). it therefore seems reasonable to assert that iconicity should be studied in the context of a culture, and that it cannot be taken for granted that results obtained from studying one group of people can necessarily be generalised to another. however, the iconicity of graphic representational systems has not previously been investigated in the context of any of south africa's many cultures. there is a need for culture-specific iconicity information in this country to enhance alternative and augmentative communication (aac) intervention for individuals with little or no functional speech (lnfs). kwazulu-natal is the province in south africa with the second highest disability prevalence rate (6.7%) (schneider et al., 1999). furthermore, the 1996 census showed that 22.9% of south africans are zulu mothertongue speakers (burger, 2000), making it one of the largest linguistic groupings in the country. picture communication symbols (johnson, 1981, 1985, 1992) is a set of aided, static communication symbols and is regarded as relatively iconic compared to other aided symbol sets and systems (mirenda ,& locke, . 1989; mizuko, 1987). although it originated in the usa, it is widely used all over the world, including south africa. an investigation into how zulu children relate to picture communication symbols (pcs) could yield valuable information on how to modify the'content, appearance or use of pcs to facilitate symbol learning and use. a review of iconicity literature revealed that one of two methodologies is normally used to determine the iconicity of symbols: either the participant is shown a the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) rouviere de waal, rene hugo, maggi soer and johann j kruger 41 symbol and asked to guess its meaning, or participants are required to match a spoken label with a symbol from a set of closed alternatives. it has been reasoned that such a forced-choice task might be easier than an openchoice task (musselwhite & ruscello, 1984), probably resulting in the best possible iconicity values. in a critique of their own study, mirenda and locke (1989) mentioned that communication overlays typically contain more than two symbols. they maintained that the inclusion of a larger number of symbols in iconicity tasks might yield more accurate results for intervention , purposes. in the light of this consideration it was decided to investigate iconicity in the context of a communication overlay. instead of presenting a participant with three to five symbols to choose from, an entire overlay was presented. the communication overlays designed by goossens', crain and elder (1996) can be photocopied directly from the manual, and are widely used unmodified in south africa. the use of a 36-matrix overlay from this collection therefore seemed appropriate for the present study. clearly such a task would differ from those in previous iconicity studies in four important ways. firstly, when participants are presented with a complete communication overlay, the set of alternatives is substantially larger than in previous studies, as has just been discussed^ secondly, all symbols are semantically related to the same theme and therefore possibly to each other, even if indirectly. thirdly, the 36 symbols comprising the set .of, alternatives will remain static across all 36 trials; and fourthly, each symbol will in time be the target symbol. these last two factors create the possibility that some participants may remember which symbols they had chosen for several consecutive trials and, in response to the next labels, narrow their selection down to those not yet chosen. the possibility that a combination of these factors might influence the iconicity values obtained should be kept in mind. ' due to the novel; methodology, the question arose as to what terminology should be used. transparency refers to 'the ease of identification of symbols when no additional cues, such as printed labels or verbal hints, are provided' (musselwhite & ruscello, 1984, p.437). in this study; however, an additional cue was provided in that the theme of the overlay was known. thus the traditional term {'transparency' could not be used. translucency on ! the ,, other . hand (typically determined by participants rating on a 5or 7point scale) · indicates the extent to which a symbol looks like its referent (blischak et al., 1997). no ratings were required from participants in the present study, so the term translucency would not apply either. it was decided that use of the more general term iconicity would be most accurate. method aim the primary aim of the study was to determine " how accurately typically developing rural zulu 10-yearolds could identify 36 picture communication symbols (pcs), presented thematically on a commercially available communication overlay, in response to spoken labels. the following objectives were formulated: to select a commercially available communication overlay, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 49, which, contained no concepts that would be foreign to rural zulu children; to determine how accurately tenyear-old zulu children would select the correct symbol in response to its spoken label; to describe error patterns; and to investigate factors that could have influenced results, specifically total frequency of selection, position on overlay and gender research design the nature of the study was exploratory. an analytical survey was conducted in which 94 rural zulu mother-tongue speakers from nine schools were exposed to 36 pcs symbols in the context of a commercially available communication overlay. in response to a verbal zulu label they had to mark the symbol they thought best depicted that concept. sampling was purposive in the sense that schools were selected according to accessibility. at the selected schools however, all children that met the selection criteria were included in the study. the data was quantitative in nature and was therefore subjected to statistical analysis to obtain iconicity values. possible influences on results and error patterns were analysed qualitatively. participants participant selection criteria participants had to be zulu mother-tongue speakers between the ages of 10 and 11, with no indication of hearing loss or uncorrected sight problems. they had to be in either grade 4 or grade 5, to ensure comparable educational and experiential backgrounds; and should never before have failed a school year. in the absence of formal assessment of mental abilities, this criterion was included to control for severe learning and mental disabilities. description of schools the kwazulu-natal department of education and culture divides the province into 196 circuits. the twenty-two primary schools in the kranskop east circuit were targeted for this study. these schools are all located along three main routes, and three schools along each route, that would be accessible by sedan vehicle, were chosen in consultation with a physical planner from the kwazulu-natal department of education and culture pietermaritzburg region. permission to perform the study was obtained from the principal of each selected school, as well as the kwazulu-natal department of education and culture. the nine schools that were selected were all co-educational government-funded schools. none were boarding schools, which means that all the participants were indigenous to the kranskop area. none of the schools had facilities for learners with special educational needs. although the mother tongue of all participants was zulu, the official language of instruction at all schools was english. the kwazulu-natal department of education and culture classifies schools as deep rural, rural, peri-urban and urban, according to no set definitions. rural is simply described as far from any town, whereas deep rural means 'off the beaten track' (p. miiller, deputy chief education specialist in education management information services, personal communication, july 9, 2001). all schools in the sample r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lize haupt and erna alant 42 were classified as rural or deep rural by the kwazulunatal department of education and culture. description of participants at the nine selected schools all children who met the selection criteria were identified. a population of 94 children were tested, of which 52 were female and 42 male. participants were between the ages of 10 and 11, and the mean chronological age was ten years and five months. procedure preparatory phase this phase included the selection and translation of a suitable communication overlay (goossens' et al., 1996), development of a test protocol, training of the research assistant and execution of a pilot study to pretest the validity of the translation and test protocol. the aim of the selection process was to minimise cultural distance between the concepts represented on the overlay and the experiential background of the target population. the basic content of the overlay had to form part of the world knowledge of children from the target population. the researcher was assisted in the selection process by a panel of three zulu judges each of whom had between four and 15 years' teaching experience in the kranskop area. to minimise the influence of linguistic factors on the performance of participants, the entire procedure was conducted in zulu. consequently all labels (the sentences/phrases accompanying the symbols on the overlay) had to be translated into zulu. the goal was to produce an ethnographic translation (brislin, 1980) and a process consisting of blind back-translation, a review committee and pre-test procedures (bracken & barona, 1991; retief, 1988) was followed. the translation process involved eight people, five of whom were zulu mother-tongue speakers with proficiency in english; and three of whom were english mother-tongue speakers with proficiency in zulii. all eight translators had previous experience with translation between the two languages. the final translations are presented in table 1. • a young zulu, adult served as research assistant. she had obtained her senior certificate and was at the time studying part time through unisa. additionally, she co-reared eight younger cousins and did pro work for two youth organizations in the kranskop area. she was selected for her proficiency in english and good rapport with young children. as an introduction to the training, the research assistant received general background information on the aims of' the study, and was presented with the communication overlay as well as the zulu and english phrases. thereafter the procedure was performed once on each of three children who met the selection criteria, but who were not included in the pilot or main studies·. these sessions were performed as part of training, and in order to develop a practical test protocol. during the first session the research assistant worked from a crude protocol designed by the researcher, and instructions were modified in consultation with the researcher where it seemed necessary. during sessions two and three the protocol did not change considerably, and the research assistant reported familiarity with the procedure. a pilot study was then performed in two phases. during the first phase the procedure was performed on ten participants who met the selection criteria, but who were not included in the main study. based on the results of this phase, minor adjustments were made to the protocol and translation. three modelled training items and three independent training items proved to be sufficient to train participants in the task, but it seemed necessary to add an instruction to scan through all the symbols before making a choice. such an instruction was added to the protocol. participants reported, that: the translation for the phrase 'let us put" on....' ('masendlale...') was too close to the translation for 'let us make the bed' ('asendlale umbhede'). consequently an alternative translation ('maseleke...'), that satisfied all the participants as well as the translators, was used, in the main study. the second phase of the pilot study entailed performing the procedure a second time on the same participants, one week after the first administration. results were compared in order to establish test-retest reliability. table 1: final translations of labels english phrase zulu translation what a mess! kwaze kwangcola! it looks like a bomb went off! sengathi kuqhume ibhomu! it is dirty. kungcolile. you need to change them. udinga ukuwashintsha." let us take it off. asikususe. help me, please. ngicela ungisize. it is finished. kuphelile. what is next? kulandelani? put it in the tub. faka kubhavu. let us make the bed. asendlale umbhede. hold this, please. ngicela ubambe lokhu. you need to pull. udinga ukudonsa. it is crooked. kugwegwile. j let us do it again. asiphinde futhi. ' fold it back. kugoqele emuva. . . , tuck it in. kushutheke. ; let us put on... maseleke... ...the sheets ...amashidi. ·, ι ...the blanket ...ingubo. ι ...the pillowcase ...iphilo. thank you. ngiyabonga. you are welcome. wamukelekile. < let me:.. . akengi... where is it? kuphi? put it here. beka lapha. puff it up. khukhumalisa. it is nice and soft. kuntofontofo. what do you think? ubona kanjani? it looks good. kubukeka kahle. / it looks bad. kubukeka kabi/ whoops! we! look at this. buka lokhu. we forgot. sikhohliwe. yes. yebo. no. cha. it is nice and clean. kuhlanzeke kahle. the south african journal of communicationdisorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) rouviere de waal, rene hugo, maggi soer and johann j kruger 43 main study the researcher and research assistant visited the nine schools and included all children that met selection criteria at every school.. sessions were conducted with ten or less participants at a time (except in two cases where twelve children were included due to time constraints) and took between 45 minutes and one hour. each participant was issued with a recording booklet (containing 36 a4 facsimiles of the communication overlay and six facsimiles of the training overlay) and a felt tip marker. the training procedure consisted of six trials where participants were asked to indicate the written word on the training overlay that corresponded table 2: presentation order of symbols with the zulu word that the research assistant read out. participants were shown and told to visually scan the entire matrix before indicating the word. since children who experience problems with sight or hearing would have difficulty with this task, it served simultaneously as hearing and visual screening. during the testing procedure, zulu labels were read in an order that was determined before commencement of the study by drawing symbols randomly from a bag and assigning each number, accordingly. participants had to mark one label per page. every label was repeated once and the procedure was not timed. after the session each participant received a token. teachers were given information on aac in general and the aims of the study, as well as worksheets for their classes. they were asked nr label in english target symbol iv nr label in english target symbol iv nr label in english target symbol iv 1 what is next? c \ 1 2 3 • • • 14 13 it is * finished. / _ _ ν t ^^^ j 6 25 puff it up. φ ν / 47 2 it is nice and soft. f \ ο \ / 7 14 let us make the bed. f v ϊμ 67 26 what a mess! 1»"" * a w 14 3 no. 0 15 thank you. f— mr 40 27 it looks like a bomb went off. f \ 32 3 no. 0 15 thank you. 40 27 it looks like a bomb went off. 32 4 you need to change them.' f \ e l · · ^ ' j 4 16 ...the blanket. v. ^ 10 28 let us do it again. f s 1 5 whoops! / ν φ 46 17 let us put on... r \ < ' s 5 29 yes. f ν © i * j 12 6 κ we forgot. f f a ) v 1 y 22 18 ...the sheets. r j o 30 put it here. k λ 13 7 what do . you think? fe) 13 19 where is it? f / 5 31 you are welcome. t \ q 9 8 it is nice and clean. ^ i ·λ 8 20 look at this. β " ν j 1 32 ...the pillow case. a 21 9 let us take it off. f \ ^ j 8 21 tuck it in. f \ l £ ] 17 33 let me... 3 10 it is -crooked. f sl l l > k. 13 22 i it is dirty. 3 34 it looks bad. r \ φ 10 10 it is -crooked. 13 22 i it is dirty. 3 34 it looks bad. 10 11 you need to pull. ( \ ν j 81 23 fold it ' back. f \ 2 35 hold this, please. t ν. 37 12 put it in the tub. ft * 57 24 help me, please. ·\ 47 36 it looks good. 0 (nr = number; iv = iconicity value) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lize haupt and erna alant 44 to discuss the session with their pupils with the help of the worksheets, as a form of debriefing. data analysis paired t-tests were performed on the two sets of data from the pilot study in order to reveal possible significant differences between the performances of the same group of participants one week apart. the performance of males and females in the main study were compared in a chi-square test. descriptive statistics, includingfrequency distribution counts, mean and standard deviation were also used. possible influences on results and error patterns were analysed qualitatively. results and discussion the results are described and discussed according to the objectives of this study. the frequency of correct responses per label is presented. an analysis of errors is presented and discussed, and possible influences on correct responses are considered. frequency of correct responses per label missing data out of the total of 3,384 responses generated in this study ! (94 participants χ 36 trials), two symbols on one page were marked on five occasions, no choice was indicated on eight occasions, and the activity graphic in the top. right hand corner of the overlay was chosen on nine occasions. in order to focus on the most salient patterns in this study, it was arbitrarily decided to consider only those responses that were indicated by at least 20% of the participants (i.e. 19 participants or more). consequently the influence of the missing data was deemed negligible. iconicity values to aid in interpretation of the results, the number as well as iconicity value of each symbol are presented in table 2. the iconicity value represents the number of participants that chose a symbol in response to its target label. doherty, daniloff & lloyd (1985) used strict (iconicity values > 75%) and lenient (iconicity values > 50%) criteria for interpreting the transparency scores of amer-ind gestures. although the present study did not investigate pure transparency, these criteria were nevertheless deemed useful. in accordance with the above criteria, one symbol (symbol 11) was found to be iconic when the strict criterion was applied, and four symbols (symbols 11; 12; 14; and 25) when the lenient criterion was applied. note that symbol 5 achieved an iconicity value of 49% (n=46) and was one response short of being classified as iconic according to the lenient criterion. thus either 2.8% or 11.1% of the symbols on the communication overlay were iconic for the participants involved, depending on the criterion used. the average of 'correct' responses across all symbols was 17.75 (18.88%) with a standard deviation of 20.17. analysis of errors when the highest frequency responses were studied for each symbol, it became clear that for some symbols many participants agreed on a single specific label, be it the target label or a non-target label. for other symbols either many possible labels, or none of the labels, were indicated. the term distinctiveness was coined to describe how well defined or specific were the evoked meanings triggered by a symbol in the mind of a viewer. this term should not be confused with 'perceptual distinctness',, as described by fuller, lloyd, and stratton (1997), which refers to the degree to which the symbols in a group are clearly different or distinct from one another. . it is also important to note that 'distinctiveness' was not intended as an equivalent to 'iconicity'. whereas both terms concern the visual relationship between a symbol and its referent, they indicate different aspects of that relationship. iconicity pertains to the degree of visual similarity perceived, as demonstrated by the use of the three dimensions, transparency, translucency and opaqueness .(blischak et al., 1997). the term distinctiveness, as used in this study, relates to the specificity of visual similarity perceived i.e. whether participants perceive similarity to one referent, to many, or to none. since iconicity and distinctiveness are not opposing terms, a symbol can be classified by both terms simultaneously. such a classification will lead to all symbols falling into one of four orthogonal groups: distinctive and more iconic (many participants chose a certain symbol in response to its target label only); indistinctive and more iconic (many participants chose a certain symbol in response to its target label, however they also chose that symbol often in response to one or more other labels); distinctive and less iconic (few participants chose a certain symbol in response to its target label, however many of them chose that symbol in response to a certain non-target label); indistinctive and less iconic (few participants chose a certain symbol in response to its target label but they chose that symbol often in response to one or more other labels). 1 the symbols from the present study were distributed across these four orthogonal groups. to determine iconicity and distinctiveness two criteria were used. regarding the iconicity of a symbol, the criterion suggested by hoemann (1975) was used (iconicity values > 25%). admittedly this criterion is very lenient (lloyci et al., 1985), but since" this analysis was concerned with relative rather than absolute iconicity, it was considered appropriate. to describe distinctiveness, all response frequencies > 20% were investigated. note that this cuttable 3: distribution of symbols according to iconicity and distinctiveness more iconic (iconicity values > 25%) less iconic (iconicity values < 25%) ^ distinctive (only one response over 20%) numbers: 5; 12; 16; 25; 27; 35 numbers: 4; 7; 13; 15; 20; 26; 28; 31 indistinctive (more than one response over 20%, · or no responses over 20%) numbers: 11; 14 numbers: 6; 18; 32; 33; 1; 2; 3; 8; 9; 10; 17; 19; 21; 22;'23; 24; 29; 30; 34; 36 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) rouviere de waal, rene hugo, maggi soer and johann j kruger 45 off point was arbitrarily selected and not statistically determined. the distribution of the symbols from the present study across these four orthogonal classifications is presented in table 3. although the variable 'distinctiveness' has never been investigated before, it was hypothesised that it would yield valuable information as to how participants viewed symbols. hence a discussion of symbols according to this classification follows, including only the most salient points. for a detailed discussion the reader is referred to haupt (2001). distinctive and more iconic six symbols (5, 12, 16, 25, 27, and 35) were classified as distinctive and more iconic. it seems that participants perceived a relatively strong visual relationship between these six symbols and their target referents, and to those referents only. it is hypothesised that of all 36 symbols included in the study these six will probably be the easiest to learn for rural zulu children. indistinctive and more iconic symbols 11 and 14 (the two symbols with the highest iconicity values) were classified as indistinctive and more iconic. it is evident that not only did participants perceive visual similarities between these symbols and their target referents, but also to other referents. it can be argued that the conceptual features of symbol 14 were exceptionally close to the theme of the overlay (making the bed). underlying similarities possibly existed between the label for this symbol and those for other symbols, making it a popular choice. it is postulated that the indistinctiveness of symbol 14 can therefore be ascribed to the context in which it was ν presented. had this symbol been presented in a group of unrelated foils like in most other iconicity studies, it probably would have scored higher on distinctiveness. the perceptual features of symbol 11 ('you need to pull') include a human figure in implied motion 'and an object linked to the figure by rope. one of the two non-target labels that were associated with this symbol by more than 20% iof participants also referred to physical motion ('let us take it off') and the selection of this symbol can therefore easily be explained. the other non-target label associated with this symbol referred to more abstract motion: from present to future ('what is next?'). the target symbol for this label (symbol 1) shows this motion with an arrow. it seems that participants preferred the symbol that implied motion by postural cues, to the symbol that included an arrow, suggesting the possibility that participants did not interpret the arrow as presenting information about movement. this phenomenon recurred throughout the analysis of error patterns and possible , causes are discussed later in the paper. another aspect to be considered is the fact that the label 'what is next?' is a question. three of the 36 symbols on the overlay contained question marks: symbols 1, 7 and 19; whereas symbol 11 did not. it is postulated that if participants recognised and optimally utilised'the question mark, they would have associated one of the three 'question mark symbols' with the label 'what comes next?'. the fact that they preferred symbol 11 may indicate that they did not interpret the question mark as indicating a question. distinctive and less iconic symbols 4, 7, 15, 28 and 31 were classified as distinctive and less iconic. symbol 4 intends to depict a change of colour, however it could also be interpreted as a change from clean to dirty, which would account for the confusion with the label 'what a mess!'. if this explanation were accepted, it would appear that participants did interpret the arrow in this symbol as indicating change. it seems once again that the clue afforded by the question mark in symbol 7 was lost since participants associated it with a label that was not in question form. to viewers that are unfamiliar with american sign language, symbols 13, 15 and 31 might be difficult to understand. symbol 15 shows two hands on the chin of a face. the label 'we forgot' was associated with this symbol, possibly because people who are shocked or surprised sometimes put their hands over their mouths. if this interpretation is accepted, participants once again did not use information afforded by the arrows pointing outwards. symbol 31 depicts two hands with empty palms turned upwards. if the hands were motionless in that position, it could be interpreted as showing that the hands are empty, possibly explaining why the label 'it is finished' was associated with this symbol. the arrows, however, imply movement away from the face, a clue that was presumably not interpreted as such by participants. symbol 28, depicted by two arrows pointing downwards, was associated with the label 'it is crooked'. the target label for symbol 28 is 'let us do it again'. the participants, instead of perceiving that the arrows indicated repetition, perceived them as two crooked lines. this finding confirms the hypothesis that participants did not interpret arrows in the symbols as indicating direction or movement. indistinctive and less iconic it is interesting to note that seven of the symbols in this classification (symbols 1, 3, 9, 19, 21, 23, 29) contain arrows. the evidence collected thus far seems to suggest that the arrows could have been a cause of the low iconicity and indistinctiveness. for example, none of the symbols indicated by this study as iconic contained arrows, and only five symbols containing arrows were not classified as indistinctive and less iconic (symbols 4, 13, 15, 20, 31). it has however already been postulated that the arrows in symbols 13, 15, 20 and 31 were not interpreted conventionally. only in symbol 4 was the arrow interpreted as indicating change. conversely it could be argued that in symbol 4 the form of the two objects stay the same and hence, unlike the other symbols containing arrows, only one attribute (colour) changes. the change is therefore highlighted, possibly contributing more to the target interpretation than the actual arrow. it seems that participants did not interpret arrows as indicating direction or change in any of the symbols except possibly one. two possible explanations for this phenomenon will be discussed. the arbitrary nature of arrows necessitates previous experience with it in order to be able to interpret it. it could be argued that children from rural areas are die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lize haupt and erna alant 46 • c o r r e c t r e s p o n s e s d t o t a l r e s p o n s e s 2 2 5 2 1 0 1 9 5 1 8 0 1 6 5 1 5 0 1 3 5 ο g 1 2 0 εγ 1 0 5 £ 9 0 7 5 6 0 4 5 3 0 1 5 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 1 0 1 1 1 2 1 3 1 4 1 5 1 6 1 7 1 8 1 9 2 0 2 1 2 2 2 3 2 4 2 5 2 6 2 7 2 8 2 9 3 0 3 1 3 2 3 3 3 4 3 5 3 6 s y m b o l n u m b e r figure i: correct responses and total frequency of selection not afforded enough opportunity to experience and learn to interpret such western conventions. yet the participants in this study all attended school where encounters with such conventions could be expected. it seems possible that either the nature or the frequency of such encounters is not sufficient in some rural schools. another possible explanation entails the predominant oral nature of the zulu culture where print traditionally played· a minor role. through the widespread use of media such as television, newspapers and billboards however, most 'oral' cultures today have had some contact with print, resulting in 'secondary orality' (ong, 1982). although most zulu people therefore are confronted with print in one way or another, there is evidence that the contact has not been great enough to promote a bookish culture among them (duncan et al., 1973;. solarsh, 2001). the possibility therefore exists that the orality of the culture, albeit secondary orality, plays a role in this phenomenon. the same argument could be made for the fact that question marks were not interpreted as indicating a question. it is not possible at this stage to determine why participants did not interpret arrows and question marks conventionally, and more research is needed. however, this phenomenon should be kept in mind when a symbol set/system is introduced to this population. special training in the use of arrows and even conventional literacy symbols might prove beneficial (moolman .& alant, 1997). ' possible influences on results consistency between sessions in order to ensure that instructions for the test procedure were consistent across sessions, the instructions of 9 out of 13 sessions (70%) were played back to one of the translators following data collection. a checklist comprising all instructions from the test protocol was used to record which instructions were used and which were left out or modified for each session. consistency was calculated by dividing the number of instructions used correctly, by the total number of instructions required for each session. an average across sessions was then calculated. accordingly, consistency across sessions was 94% (range varied between 80% and 100%). total frequency of selection of symbols the methodology required only one choice per page, to ensure that all 36 symbols were available every time a participant had to make a choice. consequently the possibility existed for a single symbol to be chosen in; response to more than one label or to none. a frequencyi procedure showing how often each participant chose ι each symbol was performed on the data. theprocedure < revealed that participants commonly selected certain1 symbols two or three times, and that in one case a symbol was selected up to nine times. it was hypothesised that the more a symbol was selected, the higher the frequency of correct responses would be. to test this hypothesis, the frequency of correct responses per symbol and the total frequency of selection per symbol were plotted on the same chart and compared (figure i). • it is evident that there is no relationship between the two sets of data to support the hypothesis. it can be concluded that the frequency of correct response's was not a function of total frequency of selection of-symbols. position of symbols on communication overlay the possibility exists that participants were influenced in their choosing of-symbols by factors such as placement on the periphery or in the centre of the overlay. it was hypothesised that symbols on the periphery could draw more attention because of less the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, .2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) rouviere de waal, rene hugo, maggi soer and johann j kruger 47 competing stimuli surrounding them. conversely it could be supposed that the four symbols in the centre of the overlay would draw more attention because viewers focus there first. a further possibility would be for participants to concentrate on the symbols in the top left quadrant of the overlay. fonseca and lassey (1964, in duncan et al., 1973) found that literate individuals preferred the top left quadrant of a page. a qualitative analysis was performed in order to reveal possible patterns visually. the analysis entailed shading the blocks of a matrix-36 with differing intensities to indicate higher and lower frequencies of selection as a function of position on the overlay. this was done once for the total frequency of selection and then again for frequency of correct responses. these results were then compared. the analysis revealed that symbols placed on the periphery, the centre and the top left quadrant of the overlay were not selected more often in total, nor more often-in response to the correct label. it seems that physical placement did not influence selection. another factor related to position could have influenced the choices of participants. to facilitate efficiency, the symbols on various communication overlays are consistently grouped according to grammatical categories namely social (pronouns, whwords, exclamation words and negative words), verbs, descriptors (adjectives and adverbs), prepositions and nouns (goossens', crain & elder, 1992). the key concept of each symbol's label serves as a basis for dividing the symbols intothese categories. symbols belonging to the same category are placed together so that they can be colour coded for easy access. each overlay is therefore roughly divided into five columns, with all social symbols placed to the very left of the overlay, followed by verbs, descriptors, prepositions, and nouns to the very right of the overlay. an informal quantitative analysis was performed in order to determine which of the categories was the most iconic. / although verbs were chosen most often on average, nouns were more often identified correctly, revealing that nouns were th'e most iconic symbols on the display. these results seem to confirm that of mizuko (1987) and bloomberg, karl|an and lloyd (1990). gender i . • · i a recent study in 'which the target population overlapped with that of the present study, revealed significant gender differences in thinking skills (solarsh, 2001). conversely, duncan et al. (1973) found that the rural zulu group they studied, performed': poorly on several measures of pictorial perception, regardless of gender, whereas results from rural tsonga, urban tsonga and urban zulu groups did reveal significant gender differences. they .hypothesised that in both tsonga groups and the urban zulu group, boys performed better than girls since it was more common for boys than for girls to attend school. however, very few children from the rural zulu group attended school leading to minimal exposure to western pictorial conventions for both genders, thus both genders performed poorly. the data of the present study was subjected to a chi square test in order to compare the number of correct responses given by boys and girls for every symbol. a significant difference was revealed for symbol 11 only, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 49, χ2(1, ν = 94) = 9.7339, ρ = 0.0018. it seems that gender did not influence the accuracy of participants in identifying pcs symbols. these results agree with the findings of duncan et al. (1973), although a different explanation for the absence of gender differences may be offered. today it is equally likely for children from both genders to attend school. the fact that both genders generally have equal opportunities for exposure to pictures and symbols possibly explains the lack of difference in performance. conclusion the iconicity of the selected pcs symbols was generally low for the population studied. this finding serves as a reminder -that although pcs had been described as one of the most iconic symbol sets (mirenda & locke, 1989; mizuko, 1987), the meanings of these symbols are still not entirely guessable for the population studied. a factor that could have contributed to low iconicity in this population was the presence of arrows in many of the symbols. it might prove profitable to use a symbol set/system that employs more postural cues and fewer arrows. alternatively, clinicians must be aware that special training in the use of arrows might be needed. in future, investigation should be made into how rural zulu mother-tongue speakers interpret arrows and why. furthermore, the unmodified use of commercially available communication overlays containing pcs symbols clearly is not ideal in the south african context. many'of the themes of the overlays and the concepts depicted on them do not promote experiential equivalence with southern african cultures. it is suggested that clinicians choose themes that are relevant to their clients, and then compile communication overlays relating to those themes and the experiential background of the client. it has been mentioned that the presentation of an array of symbols all related to the same theme, might have had an influence on iconicity and distinctiveness values. yet symbols are most often used in such a context. it is therefore argued that whatever influence these factors had on the values obtained, the influence served to make the values more functional and socially valid. it is suggested, that this methodology be considered in future iconicity studies. the construct of "distinctiveness" could hold promise for the field of aac. it should be validated and its influence on the learnability of symbols investigated. furthermore, children's perceptions of indicators like arrows need to be explored in more depth. this study should be seen as a 'first step' towards understanding iconicity in the context of a specific culture, and it clearly shows the need for such research. it is hoped that it will be instrumental in motivating others to investigate the iconicity of graphic representational systems for south african cultures before headlong implementation. acknowledgements this article is the culmination of a thesis submitted by the first author, in partial fulfillment of the requirements of the masters degree in augmentative and alternative communication, under supervision of the r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lize haupt and erna alant 48 second author. the financial assistance of the national research foundation (nrf) towards this research is hereby acknowledged. opinions expressed, and conclusions arrived at, are those of the author and are not necessarily to be attributed to the national research foundation. references blischak, d. m., lloyd, l. l., & fuller, d. r. (1997). terminology issues. in l. l. lloyd, d. r. fuller & η. h. arvidson (eds.), augmentative and alternative communication: a handbook of principles and practices (pp. 38-42). boston: allyn & bacon. bloomberg, k., karlan, g. r., & lloyd, l. l. (1990). the comparative translucency of initial lexical items represented in five graphic symbol systems and sets. journal of speech and hearing research, 33, 717-725. bracken, β. α., & barona, a. (1991). state of the art procedures, for translating, validating and using psycho-educational tests in cross-cultural assessments school psychology international, 72,119-132. brislin, r. w. (1980). translation and content analysis of oral and written materials. in h. c. triandis & j. w. berry (eds.), handbook. of crosscultural psychology: vol. 2. methodology (pp. 389-444). boston: allyn & bacon. burger, d. (ed.). (2000). south africa yearbook 2000/1. retrieved april 17, 2001, from http://www.gov.za/vearbook/rainbow.htm. deloache, j. (1991). symbolic functioning in very young children: understanding, of pictures and models. child development, 62, 736-752. deregowski, j. b. (1980a). illusions, patterns and pictures. london: academic press. deregowski, j. b. (1980b). perception. in h. c. triandes & w. lonner (eds.), handbook of cross-cultural psychology: vol. 3: basic processes (pp. 21-115). boston: allyn & bacon. doherty, j. e., daniloff, j. k., & lloyd, l. l. (1985). the effect of categorical presentation on amerind transparency. augmentative and alternative communication, 1, 10-16. duncan, h. f., gourlay, n., & hudson, w. (1973). a study of pictorial perception among bantu and white primary school children in south africa. (human sciences research council publication series no. 31). johannesburg, south africa: witwatersrand university press. fuller, d. r. (1987). effects of translucency and complexity on the associative learning of blissymbols by cognitively normal children and adults. (doctoral dissertation, purdue university,· 1987). dissertation abstracts international, 49, 71 ob. fuller, d. r. (1997). initial study into the effects of translucency and complexity on the learning of blissymbols by children and adults with normal cognitive abilities. augmentative and alternative communication, 13, 30-39. fuller, d. r., lloyd, l. l., & stratton, μ. m. (1997), aided aac symbols. in l. l. lloyd, d. r. fuller & η. h. arvidson (eds.), augmentative and alternative communication: a handbook of principles and practices (pp. 48-79). boston: allyn & bacon. goossens', c. α., crain, s., & elder, p. (1992). engineering the preschool environment for interactive, symbolic communication. birmingham, al: southeast augmentative communication publications. goossens', c. α., crain, s., & elder, p. (1996). communication displays for engineered preschool environments: book ii. matrix-32 and matrix-36 displays. solana beach, ca: mayerjohnson co. haupt, e. (2001). the iconicity of selected picture communication systems for rural zulu-speaking children. unpublished master's thesis, university of pretoria, south africa. hoemann, h. w. (1975). the transparency of meaning of sign language gestures. sign language studies, 7, 151-161. johnson, r. (1981). the picture communication symbols. solana beach, ca: mayer-johnson co. johnson, r. (1985). the picture communication symbols: book ii. solana beach, ca: mayerjohnson co. johnson, r. (1992). the picture communication symbols: book iii. solana beach, ca: mayerjohnson co. lloyd, l. l., & fuller, d. r. (1990). the role of iconicity in augmentative and alternative communication symbol learning. in w. i. fraser (ed.), key issues in mental retardation research (pp. 295-306). london: routledge. lloyd, l. l., fuller, d. r., & arvidson, η. h. (1997). augmentative and alternative communication: a handbook of principles and practices. boston: allyn & bacon. lloyd, l. l., loeding, b., & doherty, j. e. (1985). role of iconicity in sign language acquisition: a response to orlansky and bonvillian (1984). journal of speech and hearing disorders, 50, 299-301. luftig, r. l. (1983). variables influencing the learnability of individual signs and sign lexicons: a review. journal of psycholinguistic research, 12, 361-376. luftig, r. l., page, j. l., & lloyd, l. l. (1983). ratings of translucency in manual signs as a predictor of sign learnability. journal of childhood communication disorders, 6, 117134. mirenda, p., & locke, p. a. (1989). a comparison of symbol transparency in nonspeaking persons with intellectual disabilities. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 54, 131-140. mizuko, m. (1987). transparency and ease ofjearning of symbols represented by blissymbols, pcs, and picsyms. augmentative and alternative communication, 3, 129-136.. moolman, e., & alant, e. (1997). the teaching of blissymbols as a bridge into literacy for children with cognitive impairments: a comparison of two trainings approaches. the south african journal of communication disorders, 44, 73-86. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) http://www.gov.za/vearbook/rainbow.htm rouviere de waal, rene hugo, maggi soer and johann j kruger 49 musselwhite, c. r., & ruscello, d. m. (1984). transparency of three symbol communication systems. journal of speech and hearing research, 27, 436-443. ong, w. j. (1982). orality, and literacy. london: routledge. retief, a. (1988). method and theory in cross-cultural psychological assessment. (investigation into research methodology: research report nr. 6). pretoria, south africa: human sciences research council. schneider, m., claassens, m., kimmie, z., morgan, r., naicker, s., roberts, α., & mclaren, p. (1999). the extent of moderate and severe reported disability and the nature of the disability experience in south africa. pretoria, south africa: community agency for social enquiry. solarsh, b. (2001). verbal solutions of rural zulu τ speaking children to problems encountered in everyday life. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of pretoria, south africa. taylor, o. l., & clarke, m. g. (1994). culture and communication disorders: a theoretical framework. seminars in speech and language, 15, 103-114. witkin, h. a. (1967). a cognitive styles approach to cross cultural research. international journal of psychology, 2(4), 233-250. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 information for contributors 1. nature of publication the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, and critically evaluative theoretical and philosophical conceptual issues dealing· with aspects of human communication and its disorders, service provision, training and policy. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. 2. manuscript style and requirements • articles must he accompanied by a covering letter providing the author's address, telephone and fax numbers and email address. • articles must be on a4 pages in double spacing and in a font size of 12 • three print outs of the article must be submitted. • one exact copy of the article on disk must be submitted. filenames must include the first author's initials and a clearly identifiable key word and must he type-written on the last line of the last page of the reference list (for retrieval purposes only). • articles must not exceed 30 pages. • the title page of one must contain: • title of the article. • full names of the authors. • institutional affiliation. • abstract of the article in the language of the article. • the title page of the remaining two copies must not contain the authors names or institutional affiliations. • each article must contain an abstract of no more than 200 words. β all abstracts must be in english, irrespective of the language in which the article was written. • each article must provide 5-7 key words for indexing purposes. β all contributions are required to follow strictly, the style specified in the publication manual of the american psychological association (αρα pub. man., 2001). • headings are not numbered. the order of importance is indicated as follows: • main heading in capitals and bold print. • sub-headings in capitals, bold and italic print. • sub-subheadings in upper and lower case bold and italic print. • sub-sub-sub-heading in upper and lower case hold print. • major headings, where applicable, must be in the order of introduction, method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements, references. • all paragraphs should be indented. • all tables, figures and illustrations must he numbered . and provided with titles. • the title of tables, which appear above, and of figures, which appear below, must he concise but explanatory. • allow for 50-75% reduction in printing of tables, figures and illustrations. • each table, figure or illustration must appear on a separate page and be print ready. preferably not printed on colour printers. • do not include more than 10 tables, figures or illustrations. 3. references • references must be cited in the text by surname of the author and the date, e.g. van riper (1971). • where there are more than two authors, after the first occurrence, et al. may be used. • the names of all authors must appear in the reference list, which must he listed in strict alphabetical order in triple spacing at the end of the article. • all references must be included in the list, including secondary sources, (αρα pub. man. 2001). β only acceptable abbreviations of journals may be used, (see dsi-1 abstracts, october; or the world list of scientific periodicals). β the number of references should not exceed 30, unless specifically warranted. examples locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear disord., 48 339-341. penrod, j.p. (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. davis, g. & & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca: collegehill. editing • articles must be corrected for grammar and style prior to submission. • the manuscript style of the article must be strictly according to the guidelines provided. • only articles complying with the above requirements will be accepted for review. reviewing system • the peer review of refereeing system is employed as a method of quality control of this publication. • peer reviewers are selected by the editor based on their, expertise in the field and each article is sent to two independent reviewers to assess the quality of the manuscript's scientific and technical content. • the blind peer review system is employed during which the names of the author/authors are not disclosed to the reviewers. • the editor retains the final responsibility for decisions regarding revision, acceptance or rejection of the manuscript. deadline for contributions. 30lh january each year queries, correspondence & manuscripts: should he addressed to the editor, south african journal of communication disorders , south african speechlanguage-hearing association, po box 5710, the-reeds, 0158, south africa copyright the copyright of all articles printed in the south african journal of communication disorders is reserved by the south african speech-language-hearing association (saslha) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) cybernetic functioning in stuttering ursula zsilavecz μ ( l o g ) pretoria department of speech science, speech pathology and audiology, university of pretoria summary the aim of this study was to evaluate different kinds of masking noise and daf, in order to identify the condition which would elicit the highest incidence of fluency in a group of stutterers. the study demonstrates that masking noise and daf can be effectively applied as an aid in a therapy programme, viz. noise can effectively be put to use so as to encourage and reinforce somesthesia. stuttering is viewed as defective functioning in the cybernetic system. opsomming hierdie studie evalueer verskeie tipes maskering sowel as vertraagde ouditiewe terugvoering om sodoende die tipe lawaai wat die hoogste persentasie vlotheid in 'n groep hakkelaars teweegbring, te identiflseer. hierdie studie poog ook om te bewys dat maskering en vertraagde ouditiewe terugvoering effektiewelik in terapieprogramme toegepas kan word, nl. om somestetiese terugvoering te versterk. hakkel word beskou as afwykende funksionering in die kubernetiese sisteem. in an attempt to obtain a better understanding of stuttering, a number of investigators have attempted to find the cause of the problem and an appropriate "cure". many of the theories offer reasonably adequate explanations for stuttering but cures posited only provide temporary relief from stuttering. the view that the stutterer may have a malfunctioning, or a defect in the auditory feedback system and the fact that noise had an ameliorative effect on the stuttering, resulted in the development of portable devices emitting masking noise or delayed auditory feedback (daf) in an attempt to eliminate stuttering. some researchers in the field of stuttering have cautioned that it is a much more complex matter than just investigating the auditory feedback system and applying masking noise or d a f to elicit a higher percentage of fluency. this view of stuttering is limited as it does not adequately account for the disruption of the motor sequences of stuttered words. yet researchers often advise the incorporation of masking noise and d a f as a part of the therapeutic programme. van r i p e r 1 4 sums up the situation in which attempts have been made to simply alter the auditory signals by using masking noise and d a f as a clinical procedure as follows: exchanging stuttering for deafness is no great bargain and we have found little permanent improvement as a result of masking itself. van r i p e r 1 3 emphasizes therapeutic procedures involving the retraining of speech monitoring by means 'of employing auditory signals which exclude the perception of the stutterer's own speech. by means of the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c y b e r n e t i c functioning in stuttering 61 masking noise and d a f fluency can be manipulated and then reinforced. in view of previous research studies, the writer attempted to evaluate the influence of different types of masking noise and delayed auditory feedback on the speech of stutterers. the main aim was to identify the type of masking noise and/or delayed auditory feedback which would elicit the highest incidence of fluency in view of reinforcing normal motor patterns and enhancing somesthetic feedback. experimental design many different fragments and facets of information have to be brought together comprehensively to identify conclusively the most appropriate and successful type of masking device to be used in therapy as an aid so as to reinforce somesthesia. certain hypotheses can be postulated stemming from the results of previous research in this field. hypotheses the degree of fluency in stutterers can be increased by application of masking noise and daf. previous studies described devices which were developed in an attempt to ameliorate stuttering. devices were developed by trotter and l e s c h 1 2 , curlee and perkins4, dewar et al5 and van r i p e r . 1 4 . continuous masking noise and d a f elicits a higher degree of fluency than intermittent masking noise. van r i p e r 1 4 has determined that when stutterers are exposed to intermittent noise, they still tend to anticipate and expect to stutter; as a result, stuttering does in fact ensue and thus the frequency of stuttering is not diminished dramatically. these findings are in agreement with those of altrows and bryden.1 continuous masking noise obtained by using the bench model type edinburgh masker is more successful than continuous white noise as research has indicated that low frequency noise is more successful in masking the speaker's own voice (cherry and sayers3). white noise contains a wider spectrum of frequencies. persons with a severe stuttering problem will not necessarily have poorer oral perceptual abilities (jensen et al7). to a large extent previous research has been conducted in cases of severe neurological deprivation such as dysarthria or cleft palate. limited research has indicated that stutterers need not necessarily function poorly on oral perceptual tasks. subjects in view of evaluating various devices and techniques which can alter the auditory feedback signals it was necessary to select a group of subjects, representative of the population of stutterers. for the purposes of this study 14 subjects were chosen at random, all fulfilling certain criteria, thus forming a homogenous group, (see table i) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 ursula zsilavecz t a b l e i: the criteria set for the fourteen subjects d e g r e e of status subject a g e speech h o m e sex of oral defect language hearing status yrs mnths 1 21 4 v. severe afr. male normal normal 2 19 9 severe afr. male normal normal 3 37 6 severe eng. male normal normal 4 23 11 v. severe afr. male normal normal 5 20 10 severe afr. male normal normal 6 34 5 severe afr. male normal normal 7 23 2 v. severe afr. male normal normal 8 28 6 v. severe afr. female normal normal 9 30 1 v. severe afr. male normal normal 10 16 0 v. severe afr. male •normal normal 11 36 0 v. severe afr. male normal normal 12 24 6 v. severe afr. male normal normal 13 19 6 severe eng. male normal normal 14 27 9 severe afr. male normal normal e q u i p m e n t a n d a p p a r a t u s in order to compare and evaluate equipment and devices which could alter the auditory feedback signals, devices offering different kinds of auditory feedback were included in the study. equipment offering continuous masking noise including the edinburgh master (bench model — type cm3) and white noise, achieved by employing the maico, model ma.24 audiometer automatic intermittent noise was obtained by the edinburgh masker (portable model — type af1). d a f was achieved employing the 'artik,' device, similarly used in a study by martin and haroldson.8 the minimum intensity output used in the experiment was 100 db spl, for all the masking noise devices. the intensity required for d a f is the most comfortable loudness level. in the case of the study the intensity output, however, was also 100 db spl. both types of the edinburgh masker models produced a masking noise comprising a frequency of 150 hz. this meets with the requirements, discussed by cherry and sayers3, that effective masking noise comprises a low frequency — i.e. below 500 hz and that the intensity must be a minimum of 40 db above normal voice production level. the delay time of the 'artik' device was 0,13 seconds. the two tests included for oral-perceptual a b i l i t y a s similarly described by jensen et al 7 in their study were oral stereognosis and two-point discrimination. ten of the forms of the set of 25 developed by the national institute of dental· research (grossman6) were used for testing oral stereognosis. a vernier caliper, which was similarly adapted in the experiments by rutherford and mccall1 0 was used for the test of two-point discrimination.' in order to determine that all the equipment meets with the minimum the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c y b e r n e t i c functioning in stuttering intensity required for effective noise the devices were calibrated with the briiel-kjaer sound pressure level meter. experimental design and procedure the investigator drew up a comprehensive and strict plan so as to ensure that all behaviour could accurately be accounted for. the investigator saw the subjects over a period of 5 weeks. a minimum time lapse of 4i hours has to be permitted before the next experimental situation so as to avoid carry over fluency. it also suited the patients better to come to the clinic once a week. initially a videotaped recording was made of each subject's spontaneous speech under normal feedback conditions. hereafter, each subject was exposed to one of the four different experimental conditions, which were also videotaped in a soundproof room. in order to elicit an adequate sample of spontaneous speech the investigator used the "job task" as described by johnson et al and van r i p e r 1 3 the subjects were required to discuss controversial subjects so as to elicit a minimum of 400 utterances or ± 10 stuttering moments (van r i p e r 1 3 ) . after each videotaped recording was analysed and evaluated according to the "profile of stuttering severity" (van r i p e r 1 3 ) . lastly, oral stereognostic ability and twopoint discrimination was evaluated on the 14 stuttering subjects. statistical analysis the methods employed to evaluate the results were the "wilcoxon signed-rank test", also known as the "one sample wilcoxon s test . the "sign test" was employed in evaluating the results obtained on test for oral stereognosis (siegel1 1). the information for statistical analysis was obtained from each profile for each experimental condition of the subjects. the investigator had to determine the category improvement of each characteristic on the "profile of stuttering severity" (frequency, tension, duration and avoidance-postponement behaviour) when compared to the same characteristic on the profile for the control situation. results a n d conclusions it was ascertained and proved that continuous masking noise and d a f elicited the highest incidence of fluency, in comparison to automatic intermittent masking noise. with regard to the evaluation of continuous masking noise and d a f , white noise and d a f elicited more fluency, than continuous masking noise emitted by the edinburgh masker — bench model. stuttering behaviour which was effected by masking noise and dal· was fequency of stuttering, duration of the stuttering moment and tension. postponement-avoidance behaviour was hardly effected in most cases of masking noise and daf. subjective observation confirms that masking noise and dah dedie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 ursula zsilavecz creases the incidence of stuttering but does not totally ameliorate stuttering behaviour, (see table ii) t a b l e ii: a summary of the effects of the four experimental conditions on the speech of the 14 subjects characteristics of stuttering continuous noise edinburgh masker (bench model) white noise d a f automatic intermittent noise frequency slight significant dramatic slight tension slight moderate significant slight duration slight moderate dramatic slight avoidancepostponement significant moderate dramatic no the investigator observed change in vocal intensity, vocal pitch and rate of speech when the subjects were exposed to the experimental conditions. drawling of speech also occurred. it is important to note that each subject exhibited a different and personal profile under each experimental condition, thus emphasizing that therapy programmes must cater for the individual's needs. from the results in this experiment it appears that persons with a severe stuttering problem tend to. make more errors in oral stereognostic testing, than normal fluent speakers. (for this experimental condition a group of normal fluent speakers was selected to match the stutterers, as no norms are available.) persons with a severe stuttering problem have good two-point discrimination ability, they fall within the norm determined by ringel and ewanowski9 for normal fluent speakers, i.e. 1,0 mm 2 , 0 mm. it appears that subjects who achieve a greater increase of fluency with masking noise and d a f do not necessarily have good oral perceptual ability. some subjects who did not show dramatic increase of fluency when using masking noise and d a f on the contrary had good oral perceptual ability. / it has become increasingly clear that a solution for stuttering does not merely lie in altering the auditory feedback of stutterers or is developing more level and sophisticated devices. on the contrary older methods of masking auditory feedback (such as white noise) have been proved more effective.: the findings in this study also support the view that somesthetic; feedback requires attention in therapy, as well as in future research!. supplying the person who stutters with a device which produces the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c y b e r n e t i c functioning in stuttering s u f f i c i e n t high intensity noise to alter the perception of his own s d e e c h and elicit a higher incidence of fluency seems a very simple and easy way to solve the problem. unfortunately as soon as the stutterer switches off the masking device, stuttering once again increases. another disadvantage of these devices is that they do not elicit total fluency, nor are they comfortable to wear and they often elicit a negative reaction from listeners who treat a stutterer wearing a device as though he were hard of hearing. unfortunately only a limited amount of work has been done in this field and not much information is available. it is possible that stutterers tend to achieve poorer results on oral stereognosis because they are not used to employing this feedback system in speech production. from the results in this study and previous research it can be argued that a normal "fluent" speaker may also exhibit errors on this type of oral perceptual testing; the stutterer in turn may exhibit good results on oral perceptual testing. these types of tests may not truly be testing oral perceptual ability but merely tactile sensations. in other words, oral perceptual ability is not directly related to somesthesia but a separate feedback ability therefore stutterers as a group should generally compare well with "normal" fluent speakers. furthermore, this accounts„ for the phenomenon that stutterers can achieve a higher degree of fluency u n d e r specific circumstances — when given the opportunity, e.g. to use masking noise. the motor patterns required for fluent speech are available, but the stutterer has not learned to apply these in his normal speech production, as the change-over from auditory feedback to somesthetic feedback, which generally occurs during normal development has not taken place. once a higher degree of fluency has been elicited these normal motor patterns must be reinforced so that they can gradually become habitual. many "solutions" and therapy programmes fail, because these do not emphasize or reinforce awareness of normal use of fluent speech patterns. stuttering may well result from failure to learn the essential skills at the beginning of speech development. bluemel's2 comment aptly sums this up: speech which is poorly made is readily unmade. acknowledgements dr i c. uys and prof. i. s. hay, dept. of speech pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, for their invaluable guidance in preparing this study. prof. d. j. stoker, human sciences research council, pretoria, for helping with the statistical planning and analysis of the research. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 ursula zsilavecz references 1. altrows, i. and bryden, m. p. (1977): temporal factors in the effects of masking noise on the fluency of stutterers. journal of communication disorders, vol. 10, pp. 315-329. 2. bluemel, c. s. (1957): the riddle of stuttering, danville — illinois, the interstate publishing co. 3. cherry, e. c. and sayers, β. m. (1956): experiments on the total inhibition of stammering by external control and some clinical results. journal of psychosomatic research. vol. 1, pp. 223-246. 4. cur lee, r. f. and perkins, w. h. (1973): effectiveness of daf conditioning programme for adolescent and adult stutterers, beh. res. therapy. vol. 11, pp. 395-401. 5. bewar, a. et al (1976): automatic triggering of auditory feedback masking in stammering and cluttering, british journal of disorders of communication. vol. 11, 1, pp. 19-26. 6. grossman, r. c. (1967): methods of determining oral tactile experience. chap. 8. in symposium. bosma, j. f. (ed.), springfield, charles c. thomas publishers. 7. jensen, p. j. et al (1975): oral sensory-perceptual integrity of stutterers. folia phoniatrica. vol. 27, pp. 38-45. 8. martin, r. and haroldson, s. (1969): the effects of two treatment procedures on stuttering. journal of communication disorders. vol 2, pp. 115-125. 9. ringel, r. l. and ewanowski, s. j. (1963): oral perception, two-point discrimination. jshr. vol. 8, pp. 389-398. 10. rutherford, d. and mccall, g. (1967): testing oral sensation and perception in persons with dysarthria. in symposium. bosma, j. f. pp. 188-200. 11. siegel, s. (1956): nonpar'ametric statistics for the behavioural sciences. new york, mcgraw-hill. 12. trotter, w. d. and lesch, μ. m. (1967): personal experiences with a stutter aid. jshd. vol. 32, 3, pp. 271-272. 13. van riper, c. (1971): the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, n. j., prentice hall. 14. van riper, c. (1973): the treatment of stuttering. englewood cliffs, n. j., prentice-hall. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a u d i o m e t e r s i m p e d a n c e m e t e r s light, portable, screening and clinical, manual and automatic phonic ear f.m. t e a c h i n g s y s t e m s w i t h phonak audio-input h e a r i n g a i d s needler westdene organisation (pty) ltd. p . o . b o x 2 8 9 7 5 s a n d r i n g h a m 2 1 3 1 j o h a n n e s b u r g t e l e p h o n e ( 0 1 1 ) 6 4 0 5 0 1 7 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 35 antisiperende labiale koartikulasie: 'n elektromiografiese studie by afrikaanssprekendes karin theron en anita van der merwe departement kommunikasiepatologie universiteit van pretoria malcolm baker departement neurologie 1 militire hospitaal pretoria opsomming die verskynsel van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie word bestudeer om inligting te bekom aangaande die motoriese beplanning van spraak deur die brein. die doel van die studie is om inligting te bekom in verband met die temporale omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie, die verloop van koartikulasie vir uitinge wat bestaan mt twee geronde vokale geskei deur 'n nie-labiale konsonant en laastens die effek van 'n woordgrens op die aanvang van koartikulasie by normale afrikaanssprekende volwassenes. die metode het bestaan uit die opname van elektromiografiese aktiwiteit vanaf musculus orbicularis oris met 'n bipolere konsentriese naaldelektrode. die resultate dui daarop dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie wel voorkom en dat die aanvang van lipronding toenemend vroeer intree namate die duur van die konsonantstring wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan toeneem. by die ondersoek van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie m uitings wat bestaan uit twee geronde vokale wat deur 'n nie-labiale konsonant geskei word, is gevmd dat twee pieke van elektromiografiese aktiwiteit met 'n duidelike afname in aktiwiteit tussen die pieke voorkom. laastens is gevmd dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie oor woordgrense kan voorkom. die resultate word bespreek aan die hand van verskiuende teoretiese modelle van spraakproduksie wat poog om die verskynsel van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie te verklaar. abstract anticipatory coarticulation sheds light on the planning of speech by the brain. the aim of this study is to gain information on the temporal extent of anticipatory labial coarticulation, the course of coarticulation for utterances consisting of two rounded vowels separated by ah intervening consonant without rounding specification and lastly the effect of word boundaries on the onset of coarticulation iri normal afrikaans speaking adults. the method consisted of the recording of electromwgraphic (emg) activity from musculus orbicularis oris using a bipolar concentric needle electrode. the results suggested that anticipatory coarticulation is* demonstrated by afrikaans speaking individuals and that the onset of liproundmg for a rounded vowel commenced earlier when the duration of the nonldbial consonantal string preceding the rounded vowel increased during the investigation of anticipatory labial coarticulation in utterances consisting of two rounded vowels separated by a nonlabial consonant, it was found that two peaks in emg activity with a pronounced decrease m activity between the peaks occurred. lastly it was found that anticipatory coarticulation can occur across word boundaries. the results are discussed in relation to different theoretical models of speech production which have been proposed to account for the phenomenon of anticipatory labial coarticulation. sleutelwoorde: antisiperende koartikulasie, elektromiografiese aktiwiteit, motoriese beplanning, teoretiese modelle van spraakproduksie beplanning van spraak word nog dikwels as 'n erisms", dat 'n spreker 'n volledige frase in gereedheid het hipotetiese en kontroversiele konsep beskou, omdat daar voordat dit geuiter word (hams 1984; borden & hams, nie 'n objektiewe meetinstrument bestaan wat beplanning 1984). antisiperende koartikulasie reflekteer die omvang kan meet nie. baie bewyse word egter aangevoer vir die van vooruitbeplanning van komende segmente (katz, voorafbeplanning van spraak. by normale sprekers 1988a; 1988b) en aangesien koartikulasie ekstern suggereer onbewuste woorden foneemomruilings, "spoongemanifesteer word, is dit meetbaar en word dit bestudeer die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 karin theron, anita van der merwe en malcolm baker om inligting te verskaf aangaande die grootte en aard van die eenhede wat as invoer vir die motoriese beplanning van spraak gebruik word (kent & minifie, 1977; lubker, 1981; lubker & gay, 1982; katz, machetanz, schonle & orth, 1990). studies aangaande antisiperende labiale koartikulasie is tot op datum in tale soos sweeds, russies, frans en engels uitgevoer, maar daar bestaan nog 'n leemte t.o.v. die bestudering van hierdie verskynsel in afrikaans. aangesien daar nog baie kontroversie in die literatuur bestaan omtrent sekere kernaspekte van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie is verdere studies nodig om hierdie aspekte te ondersoek en om die universaliteit daarvan te bepaal. verder is dit noodsaaklik om antisiperende koartikulasie by normaalsprekende persone te bestudeer, want eers wanneer 'n verwysingsraamwerk van normale koartikulasie daargestel is kan neuromotoriese afwykings, veral in die beplanning van spraak, binne hierdie raamwerk bestudeer word. antisiperende koartikulasie word as 'n inherente deel van die neuromotoriese beplanning van spraak beskou. die beplanning veroorsaak dat die artikulators geaktiveer word en sodoende kan 'n artikulatoriese doelwit van 'n komende segment bereik word voordat dit akoesties gerealiseer word (sussmann, marquardt, macneilage & hutchinson, 1988). borden en harris (1984) definieer koartikulasie as die temporale oorvleueling van artikulatoriese bewegings vir verskillende foneme. antisiperende labiale koartikulasie verwys spesifiek na die vroee aanset van kenmerke wat verband hou met labialiteit, bv. die aanset van ronding vir geronde vokale (lubker & gay, 1982). die temporale en ruimtelike aspekte van linguale, labiale, velere en mandibulere koartikulasie is al vanaf die 1960's bestudeer. die eksperimentele metode behels gewoonlik die kontrolering van een of twee iisiologiese aanduiders van neuromuskulere aktiwiteit, tesame met die ouditief waarneembare spraaksein. die iisiologiese metings word verkry deur bv. kineradiografiese ontleding (bv. verplasing van die tong of velum) of elektromiografiese (emg) metings vanaf die toepaslike agonisspier(e) verantwoordelik vir die artikulatoriese beweging (bv. die orbicularis oris vir lipronding) (sussmann et al., 1988; blair & smith, 1986). een van die aspekte van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie wat al dikwels bestudeer is, is die temporale omvang van die antisipering. deur die aspek te bestudeer word inligting ingewin aangaande die grootte en aard van die eenhede wat die brein gebruik in die motoriese beplanning van spraak, asook inligting aangaande die kontrole en ordening van die eenhede (lubker, 1981; lubker & gay, 1982; katz, 1988a; 1988b; katz et al., 1990). daar is tans twee opponerende sienings aangaande die temporale omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie. die een groep navorsers meen dat 'n persoon labiale beweging vir 'n geronde vokaal beplan in verhouding tot die duur van die foneme wat dit voorafgaan (daniloff & moll, 1968; benguerel & cowan, 1974; lubker, 1981; sussmann & westbury, 1981). volgens hierdie navorsers kan antisiperende labiale koartikulasie 'n redelik wye omvang he. benguerel en cowan (1974) het gevind dat lipprotrusie vir franse geronde vokale soveel as ses konsonante voor die akoestiese aanset van die geronde vokaal voorkom. daniloff en moll (1968) het weer gevind dat lipronding in engels tot vier konsonante voor die akoestiese aanset van die geronde vokaal 'n aanvang neem. waar die bogenoemde navorsers die hoeveelheid konsonante bepaal het waaroor antisiperende labiale koartikulasie kan strek, het lubker (1981) die tydsverloop in millisekondes bepaal vanaf die aanset van emg aktiwiteit geassosieer met lipronding tot by die akoestiese aanset van die geronde vokaal. hy het gevind dat labiale emg aktiwiteit vir sweedse geronde vokale tot 600ms voor die akoestiese aanvang van die geronde vokaal kan begin (mcallister, lubker & carlson, 1974; lubker, 1981). die wye omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie impliseer dat die brein 'n bespiedingsproses uitvoer op die uiting wat geproduseer gaan word. hierdie vermoe van die brein om so 'n bespiedingsmeganisme te implementeer tydens die motoriese beplanning van spraak is deur henke (in kent & minifie, 1977) gepostuleer in 'n spraakproduksiemodel wat as henke se bespiedingsmodel bekend staan. henke spesifiseer egter nie die temporale omvang van die bespiedingsgebeure nie en meen dat 'n komende kenmerk soos ronding deur εύ die foneme wat die geronde klank voorafgaan, geantisipeer sal word, solank die voorafgaande kenmerke nie antagonisties is t.o.v. die bepaalde kenmerk nie. die ander groep navorsers postuleer dat antisiperende koartikulasie nie so 'n wye omvang kan he nie en dat die aanset van die rondingsbeweging geassosieer met die geronde vokaal beperk is tot 'n relatief klein temporale venster van ongeveer 250ms (bell-berti & harris, 1979; 1981; 1982). hierdie aanname impliseer dat lipronding op 'n vasgestelde tyd voor die aanvang van 'n geronde vokaal 'n aanvang sal neem. die aanvang van die lipronding sal dus nie wissel na gelang van die lengte van die konsonantstring wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan nie (gay 1979; bell-berti & harris, 1979; 1981; 1982). die kontroversie wat gegenereer word deur die opponerende sienings is van besondere belang in spraaknavorsing, aangesien dit verskillende verduidelikings van die motoriese organisasie van antisiperende koartikulasie gee (lubker & gay, 1982). die eerste siening impliseer dat die brein die motoriese kontroleprogram vir die aanset van labiale aktiwiteit versigtig beplan en aanpas by die duur van die konsonantstring wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan. laasgenoemde siening onderskryf dus die werking van 'n bespiedingsmeganisme tydens die motoriese beplanning van spraak. die tweede siening postuleer die bestaan van 'n vaste en relatief konstante (dalk 250 ms) "tydsvenster" waar die brein nie genoodsaak word om enige fyn aanpassings t.o.v. temporale aanset te maak nie. wanneer 'n labiale rondingsbeweging benodig word, word dit op 'n vasgestelde tyd voor die geronde vokaal gei'nisieer en die duur van die voorafgaande konsonantstring is dus irrelevant (lubker & gay, 1982). nog 'n aspek van antisiperende koartikulasie wat dikwels bestudeer word, is die voorkoms van koartikulasie vanaf een geronde vokaal na 'n ander waar die twee vokale geskei word deur tussenkomende nie-labiale konsonante wat nie met ronding geproduseer word nie (v k n l v konteks). in sulke gevalle het albei bogenoemde, opponerende groepe navorsers gevind dat 'n keep in die elektromiografiese aktiwiteit voorkom. twee duidelike pieke in emg aktiwiteit met 'n totale staking of noemenswaardige vermindering in emg-aktiwiteit tussen die pieke word in die foneemkonteks onderskei. die keepverskynsel impliseer dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie afwesig is in die vgkn]vg-konteks. indien antisiperende labiale koartikulasie kan voorkom oor the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) labiale koartikulasie: ' elektromiografiese studie by afrikaanssprekendes 37 f o n e e m s t r i n g e van t o t v y f o f s e s konsonante (benguerel & cowan, 1974) ontstaan die vraag hoekom dit nie volgehou kan w o r d in /vkv/-uitinge nie, waar v=/u/ en die vokale deur slegs een konsonant geskei word. navorsers wat meen dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie beperk is tot 'n relatief klein temporale venster en dat die labiale rondingsbeweging dus op 'n vasgestelde tyd voor die aanvang van die geronde vokaal 'n aanvang neem, meen dat die keepof dubbele piekverskynsel (lubker & gay, 1982) bewys lewer vir hulle aanname. navorsers wat egter die werking van 'n bespreidingsmeganisme ondersteun, p o s t u l e e r dat die keepverskynsel slegs daarop dui dat idiosinkratiese aanpassings in beplanning nodig is, afhangende van die eerste vokaal in die segmentreeks (m.a.w. die vokaal wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan) (sussmann & westbury, 1981). die afname in lipronding tydens die produksie van die konsonant voor die tweede geronde vokaal staan dus teenoor die werking van die bespiedingsmodel vir s p r a a k p r o d u k s i e soos voorgestel deur henke (in k e n t & minifie, 1977; bell-berti & harris, 1974; gay, 1979). 'n ander aspek van antisiperende koartikulasie wat al dikwels bestudeer is, is die invloed van 'n woordgrens op die aanvang van koartikulasie. sommige navorsers het gevind dat die woordgrens nie die aanvang van die rondingsbeweging bei'nvloed nie (daniloff & moll, 1968; benguerel & cowan, 1974; bell-berti & harris, 1982). ander het weer gevind dat sekere linguistiese grense wel 'n effek het (mclean, 1973). 'n spraakproduksiemodel, wat o.a. poog om die verskynsel van antisiperende koartikulasie te verklaar, is deur kozhevnikov en chistovich (in kent & minifie, 1977) voorgestel. die navorsers voer aan dat die invoereenheid vir die motoriese beplanning van spraak 'n artikulatoriese lettergreep is, wat bestaan uit enige hoeveelheid konsonante gevolg deur 'n vokaal, en dat die lettergreepgrens 'n beperking plaas op antisiperende koartikulasie. indien koartikulasie egter nie deur die woordof lettergreepgrens bei'nvloed word nie, kan die artikulatoriese lettergreepmodel van kozhevnikov en chistovich (in kent & minifie, 1977) nie sonder meer aanvaar word nie. 'n verdere afleiding wat gemaak kan word indien die woordgrens nie die aanvang van antisiperende koartikulasie bei'nvloed nie, is dat die brein se linguisties-simboliese beplanning van spraak afsonderlik van die motoriese beplanning van 'n uiting plaasvind (van der merwe, 1986). dit is dus duidelik dat daar 'n behoefte bestaan aan verdere ondersoek van sekerej aspekte van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie, aangesien daar steeds kontroversie in die literatuur heers oor die voorkoms en aard daarvan. verder is geen studies m.b.t. antisiperende labiale koartikulasie al met afrikaanssprekende persone uitgevoer nie en is dit dus nodig om hierdie verskynsel te ondersoek ten einde te bepaal of die voorkoms en aard daarvan universeel is. metode doelstellings die doel van die studie is om inligting te bekom aangaande spesifieke aspekte van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie by afrikaanssprekende normale sprekers, ten einde afleidings te maak aangaande die motoriese beplanning van spraak deur die brein. die volgende aspekte word ondersoek: ' die temporale omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie. die doel is dus om te bepaal of die aanvang van labiale beweging, geassosieer met ronding, beplan word in verhouding tot die duur van die nie-labiale konsonantstring wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan (wye omvang) en of die labiale rondingsbeweging telkens 'n vasgestelde tyd voor die aanvang van die geronde vokaal gemisieer word (beperkte omvang). hierdie aspek sal dus inligting verskaf aangaande die grootte en aard van die invoereenheid wat die brein gebruik in die motoriese beplanning van spraak. die verloop van elektromiografiese (emg) aktiwiteit vir uitinge wat bestaan uit twee geronde vokale wat deur 'n nie-labiale . konsonant geskei word (vgkn,v konteks). verskeie navorsers (gay, 1979; bell-berti & harris, 1982) het gevind dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie in hierdie foneemkonteks afwesig is. laasgenoemde bevinding sal dus die werking van 'n bespiedingsmeganisme tydens die motoriese beplanning van spraak teenstaan. die effek van woordgrense op die aanvang van die labiale rondingsbeweging. daar word eerstens bepaal of antisiperende labiale koartikulasie oor woordgrense sal voorkom en of 'n woordgrens die voorkoms van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie gedurende die eerste woord van 'n tweewoorduiting inhibeer soos gepostuleer deur kozhevnikov en chistovich (in kent & minifie, 1977). vervolgens word bepaal of die aanvangsmoment van ronding bei'nvloed word deur die woordgrense vir uitinge waar 'n sekere konsonantkombinasie binne 'n woord en oor 'n woordgrens voorkom. navorsingsontwerp die onderskeie doelstellings het telkens dieselfde eksperimentele ontwerp. dieselfde respondente is vir al drie die ondersoeke gebruik. die ontwerp bestaan telkens uit een of twee eksperimentele materiaalgroepe met 'n kontrole materiaalgroep (guy, edgley, arafat & allen, 1987). die eksperimentele materiaalgroep verwys na uitings waarin 'n geronde vokaal voorkom. sekere handelings is met die eksperimentele, sowel as die kontrole materiaalgroep uitgevoer, waar die handeling telkens bestaan het uit die lees van 'n spesifieke uiting vanaf 'n kaartjie. die kontrole materiaalgroep verwys na uitings waarin daar nie 'n geronde vokaal voorgekom het nie. die1 kontrolie materiaalgroep was telkens soortgelyk aan die eksperimentele materiaalgroep, maar in die plek van die geronde vokaal het die kontrole materiaalgroep die hoe voorvokaal /i/, wat met lipstrekking geproduseer word, bevat. die kontrole materiaalgroep is ingesluit om te kontroleer dat labiale emg-aktiwiteit wat tydens die produksie van die eksperimentele materiaalgroep opgeteken word, wel voorkom a.g.v. die teenwoordigheid van die geronde vokaal. die onderskeie doelstellings en handelings toegepas binne die eksperimentele en kontrole materiaalgroepe verskyn in tabel 1. proefpersone die proefpersone het 5 normaalsprekende persone sonder enige geskiedenis van spraakof neurologiese afwykings ingesluit. die proefpersone moes oor normale gehoor en intelligensie beskik en tussen die ouderdomme 19 en 25 jaar wees. almal was sprekers van standaard afrikaans. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 karin theron, anita van der merwe en malcolm baker tabel 2 verskaf 'n opsommende weergawe van die kenmerke van die proefpersone. apparaat vir die opname van die elektromiografiese (emg) potensiale vanaf musculus orbicularis oris is gebruik gemaak van 'n bipolere konsentriese naaldelektrode wat gekoppel is aan kanaal 1 van 'n multikanaal elektromiograaf, medelec mystro ms25. daar is gebruik gemaak van 'n bipolere naaldelektrode, aangesien die tipe elektrode emg-aktiwiteit binne 'n baie klein gebied kan bepaal. die spraaksein is opgeneem deur 'n mikrofoon, pro 2 dynamic microphone met 'n impedans van 300u wat aan kanaal 2 van die elektromiograaf gekoppel is en 'n visuele voorstelling van die spraaksein tot gevolg gehad het. daar is spesiaal deur 'n tegnikus 'n spraakprogram vir opname van die akoestiese sein, gei'nstalleer. die medelec mystro ms25 besit 'n funksie vir die direkte stoor van resultate. die elektromiografiese aktiwiteit, op kanaal 1, en die visuele voorstelling van die spraaksein, op kanaal 2, is onder mekaar op die rekenaarskerm van die elektromiograaf vertoon (figuur 1). eksperimentele en kontrole materiaal motivering vir die keuae van apraakmateriaal eksperimentele materiaal die eksperimentele materiaal wat geselekteer is vir die uitvoering van die studie het bestaan uit onsinreekse wat opgestel is om aan die doel van die studie te voldoen. die reekse is elkeen vyf keer agtereenvolgend gese en die elektromiograaf is geprogrammeer om telkens die gemiddeld van elke uiting voor te stel vir beide die spraaksein en die emg-aktiwiteit. die gemiddeld van vyf herhalings verhoog die betroubaarheid van die resultate. al die materiaal is so opgestel dat die uiting met 'n stemhebbende klank begin en dat die konsonante wat die stemhebbende geronde vokaal voorafgaan, stemloos is. sodoende kon daar tydens data analise maklik op die visuele voorstelling van die spraaksein onderskei word tussen die konsonantstring en die aanvang van die geronde vokaal (figuur 1). kontrole materiaal die kontrole materiaal is opgestel om te bepaal of labiale emg-aktiwiteit, voorkom in segmentreekse wat nie 'n tabel 1: doelstellings en handelings toegepas binne die kontrole en eksperimentele materiaalgroepe. doelstellings eksperimentele kontrole materiaalgroep materiaal groep groep 1 groep 2 bepaling van die temporale omvang van /isu/ /is=>/ /isi/ antisiperende labiale koartikulasie in uitinge met /istu/ /isto/ /isti/ verskillende konsonantstringduur /istsu/ /istso/ /istsi/ /iststu/ /iststz)/ /iststi/ /ikststu/ /ikststo/ /ikststi/ bepaling van die verloop van elektromiografiese /usu/ /isi/ aktiwiteit in die v k . v -konteks g nl g /uku/ /iki/ bepaling van die effek van woordgrense op die /gistul/ /dis//tul9/ /gisti/ aanvang van die labiale rondingsbeweging /gistut/ /disti/ ! /gistup/ /dis/ /tutors/ ! /gistof/ /dis/ /tupa/ 1 /gistol/ 1 /gistom/ /dis/ /toils/ /dis//tol9/ 1 1 1 /dis/ /tomi/ 1 proefpersoon ouderdom geslag suiwertoondrempels dbl/dbr spreektaal 1 21 vroulik 5/10 afrikaans y 2 19 vroulik 10/10 afrikaans 3 24 vroulik 15/10 afrikaans 4 24 manlik 5/5 afrikaans 5 20 manlik 10/10 afrikaans tabel 2: gegewens omtrent die proefpersone the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) labiale koartikulasie: ' elektromiografiese studie by afrikaanssprekendes 39 g e r o n d e vokaal bevat nie. die kontrole segmentreekse is s o o r t g e l y k aan die wat in die eksperimentele ondersoek in kombinasie met 'n geronde vokaal gebruik word en is ook telkens vyf maal herhaal. indien hierdie betrokke s e g m e n t r e e k s e nie tydens produksie met lipronding g e p a a r d gaan nie, kan aanvaar word dat labiale e m g a k t i w i t e i t wat voorkom in die toetsmateriaal wat wel 'n geronde vokaal bevat, verband hou met die geronde vokaal alleen (figuur 2). kontrolemateriaal sal telkens na die e k s p e r i m e n t e l e materiaal beskryf word. beekryiving van spraakmateriaal spraakmateriaal om die invloed van die duur van die konsonantstring op die aanvang van labiale emg aktiwiteit te bepaal die konsonantreekse wat in die studie gebruik word om die invloed van die duur van die konsonantstring (konsonantstringtyd) op die aanvangsmoment van labiale emg-aktiwiteit (antisiperingstyd) te bepaal, is gekies n.a.v. 'n soortgelyke studie wat in sweeds uitgevoer is (lubker, 1981). die geronde vokale is egter gekies n.a.v. afrikaanse vokale wat in afrikaans gespesifiseer is t.o.v. ronding. die kontrole en eksperimentele materiaal om die invloed van die duur van die konsonantstring op die aanvang van labiale emg-aktiwiteit te bepaal, het bestaan uit onsin segmentreekse in die vorm: /v^kv,/. vj verwys na die hoe voorvokaal /i/ wat met 'n gespreide lipposisie geproduseer word en dus nie sal inmeng met labiale ronding nie. ko verwys na die konsonantstring bestaande uit 1-5 nie-labiale konsonante en v 2 verwys in die geval van die kontrolemateriaal na die hoe voorvokaal /i/ en in die geval van die eksperimentele materiaal na die geronde vokaal /z>/ of/u/. twee geronde vokale is ingesluit ten einde die hoeveelheid data te vermeerder en te bepaal of die twee geronde vokale 'n ooreenstemmende effek het. die spesifieke uitings wat vir die eksperimentele en kontrole materiaalgroepe gebruik is verskyn in tabel 1. spraakmateriaal om die verloop van emg-aktiwiteit te ondersoek vir uitings wat bestaan uit twee geronde vokale wat deur 'n nie-labiale konsonant geskei word (vjkjv konteks) die eksperimentele materiaal het telkens bestaan uit twee geronde vokale wat deur 'n nie-labiale konsonant geskei is. om te kontroleer dat labiale emg-aktiwiteit wel voorkom a.g.v. die voorkoms van die geronde vokaal en dat die konsonante /s/ en /k/ in die betrokke uitings nie met liprondingsaktiwiteit geproduseer word nie, is k2: kanaal 2 spraaksein x: konsonantstringtyd die stemlose deel van die uiting y: antisiperingstyd die afstand vanaf die aanvang van emg-aktiwiteit tot die akoestiese aanset van die geronde vokaal a: kursor 1 geplaas op die aanvang van die konsonantstring b: kursor 2 geplaas op die akoestiese aanvang van die geronde vokaal figuur 1: rekenaaruitdruk vanaf medelec mystro ms25 ter illustrasie van konsonantstringtyd, antisiperingstyd en optekenkanale. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 kontrole-uitings saamgestel. die spesifieke uitings wat vir die eksperimentele en kontrole materiaalgroepe gebruik is verskyn in tabel 1. spraakmateriaal om die invloed van woordgrense op die aanvang van labiale emg aktiwiteit te bepaal uitings is saamgestel wat as een of twee woorde uitgespreek moes word. die konsonantkombinasie/st/het beide binne 'n woord en oor 'n woordgrens voorgekom (benguerel & cowan, 1974). die uitings is telkens met 'n nie-labiale konsonant begin sodat daar tydens produksie van die klank nie met lipronding ingemeng word nie. uitings is telkens met die geronde vokaal /u/en/3/opgestel ten einde die hoeveelheid data te vermeerder en te bepaal of die twee geronde vokale 'n ooreenstemmende effek het. dit was egter moeilik om sinvolle uitings te vind wat aan die vereistes wat die metings stel voldoen. daar is gepoog om veral die uitings wat as twee woorde uitgespreek moes word sinvol te hou. twee van hierdie ses uitings was egter sinloos wat dus lei tot kunsmatige woordgrense. hierdie feit sal egter by die interpretasie van die resultate in gedagte gehou word. om te bepaal of die rondingsbeweging oor die woordgrens voorgekom het, m.a.w. of die beweging alreeds gedurende die eerste woord gei'nisieer is, is gebruik gemaak van groep β uitings, aangesien hierdie uitings as twee woorde uitgespreek is. twee van die uitings met die vokaal /u/ was ongelukkig onsinuitings, terwyl die res van die uitings sinvolle woorde was. daar kon dus so bepaal word of die rondingsbeweging oor die woordgrens van sinvolle en onsinuitings voorkom, of nie. om te bepaal of die karin theron, anita van der merwe en malcolm baker woordgrens die aanvang van ronding beduidend be'invloed het vir uitings wat onderskeidelik as een en twee woorde uitgespreek is, waar die konsonantkombinasie /st/ onderskeidelik binne die woord en oor die woordgrens voorkom" (benguerel & cowan, 1974) is gebruik gemaak van groep a en groep β uitings. eksperimentele materiaal: groep a /gistul/ groep β /dis/ /toeia/ /gistut/ /dis/ /toet3rs/ /gistup/ /dis/ /tupa/ /gistoff /dis//tofis/ /gistol/ /dis/ /tol3/ /gistom/ /dis/ /tomi/ kontrole materiaal: die kontrole-uitings is opgestel om te kontroleer dat die konsonante /g/, /d/, /s/ en itl wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan nie met lipronding geproduseer word nie. die geronde vokaal wat in die toetsmateriaal voorgekom het, is vervang met die hoe voorvokaal ν wat met 'n gespreide lipposisie geproduseer word en dus nie met lipronding sal inmeng nie. die kontrole-uitings het dus bestaan uit die deel van die toetsmateriaal wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan en die ho6 voorvokaal /i/, omdat slegs hierdie dele van die uitings vir analise gebruik is. die kontrole uitings was soos volg: /gisti/ /disti/ 4k 2 1 l / i s t s figuur 2: rekenaaruitdruk vanaf medelec mystro ms25 ter illustrasie van die afwesigheid van emgaktiwiteit tydens die produksie van 'n kontrole-uiting the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t · perende labiale koartikulasie: 'n elektromiografiese studie by afrikaanssprekendes prosedure vir opname 41 nie n a v o r s e r is bygestaan deur 'n neuroloog wat ervare • die uitvoering van elektromiografiese ondersoeke. 'n r nolsre n a a l d e l e k t r o d e is in die midlyn van die musculus nh'cularis oris, bekend as die agonis vir lipronding (bellr^ti & harris, 1979) geplaas. die elektromiografiese ktiwiteit geassosieer met lipronding van die spier is op k naal een van die elektromiograaf opgeneem. die skrofoon is op 'n afstand van 5-10 cm vanaf die spreker e mond geplaas en die spraaksein is op kanaal twee van die e l e k t r o m i o g r a a f opgeteken. die gemiddelde van die vyf duksies van elke uiting vir die elektromiografiese a k t i w i t e i t en die visuele voorstelling van die spraaksein is onder mekaar op die ingeboude rekenaarskerm vertoon. 'n g r o n d e l e k t r o d e op die persoon se linkerwang is gebruik om enige vreemde elektriese stroom te ontvang en gevolglike artefakte uit te skakel. die eksperimentele materiaal is op kaartjies van 10 χ 4 cm geskryf en die kaartjies is geskommel sodat e k s p e r i m e n t e l e materiaal wat dieselfde aspek toets nie ηά mekaar voorgekom het nie. hierdeur is gepoog om die moontlike invloed wat die woorde op mekaar kon he uit te skakel. die kontrole materiaal is eerste geproduseer en indien liprondingsaktiwiteit tydens die produksie van die kontrole materiaal teenwoordig was, is die persoon nie in die studie gebruik nie. dit het wel gebeur dat een persoon as gevolg van hierdie rede nie by die studie ingesluit is nie. die proefpersone is vooraf elkeen geleentheid gegee om die uitspraak van die uitinge te oefen. analise van data tydens analise van die resultate is elke persoon se data wat op 'n rekenaardisket gestoor is, herroep. die elektromiografiese aktiwiteit op kanaal een en die spraaksein op kanaal twee vir die betrokke uiting het dan onder mekaar op die rekenaarskerm verskyn. deur middel van twee kursors kon die navorser dan die tydsverloop tussen die verlangde punte meet. soos die kursors beweeg is, het die tydsverskil tussen die twee kursors in millisekondes op die rekenaarskerm verskyn. die amplitude van 'n sekere punt kon jook bepaal word d.m.v. die plasing van die kursor op daardie punt. die amplitude in u v vir 'n spesifieke plasirig het ook onder aan die rekenaarskerm verskjm. j i analise van data om die invloed van die duur van die konsonantstring (konsonantstringtyd) op die aanvang van labiale emg-aktiwiteit (antisiperingstyd) te bepaal vir elke uiting is die duur van die konsonantstring in millisekondes gemeet (konsonantstringtyd) deur die kursors op die begin en einde van die konsonantstring, m.a.w. die stemlose deel van die uiting te plaas. die analise is uitgevoer op kanaal 2, m.a.w. die spraaksein. vervolgens is die tydsverloop in millisekondes bepaal vanaf die aanvang van emg-aktiwiteit (kanaal 1) tot die akoestiese aanset van die geronde vokaal (kanaal 2) (antisiperingstyd). die begin van emg-aktiwiteit is beskou as die eerste duidelike uitwyking vanaf die basislyn soos beoordeel deur die navorser en die neuroloog (figuur 1). die verband tussen antisiperingstyd en konsonantstringtyd is bepaal deur staltistiese metodes soos by die verwerking van data beskryf word. die oogmerk was dus om te bepaal of antisiperingstyd wissel na gelang die duur van die konsonantstring wissel. analise van data om koartikulasie van een geronde vokaal na 'n ander te ondersoek (v^nlvg-konteks) by die analise van die data is die verloop van die emgaktiwiteit nagegaan. daar is gekyk of daar twee piekperiodes voorkom met 'n duidelike afname in aktiwiteit tussen die twee piekperiodes. indien twee piekperiodes onderskei kan word, is die maksimum amplitude in mikrovolt van die twee piekperiodes gemeet d.m.v. die kursors op die rekenaarskerm. soos die kursors beweeg is, het die amplitude in mikrovolt aan die onderkant van die skerm verskyn. die minimum amplitude van die keep tussen die twee piekperiodes is ook bepaal. die verloop van die emg-aktiwiteit is subjektief beoordeel deur die navorser en die neuroloog. analise van data om die invloed van woordgrense op die aanvang van labiale emg aktiwiteit te bepaal • die materiaal is so opgestel dat die konsonante wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan stemloos is en dus maklik onderskeibaar is van die stemhebbende geronde vokaal op die visuele voorstelling van die spraaksein. die duur van die konsonantstring in millisekondes is bepaal asook die tydsverloop vanaf die aanvang van emgaktiwiteit tot die aanvang van die geronde vokaal. bogenoemde is gemeet vir beide groep a (uitings wat as een woord uitgespreek is) en groep β (uitings wat as twee woorde uitgespreek is). verwerking van data verwerking van data omdie invloed van die duur van die konsonantstring (konsonantstringtyd) op die aanvang van labiale emgaktiwiteit (antisiperingstyd) te bepaal puntediagramme is afsonderlik vir elke proefpersoon vir eksperimentele groep 1 en groep 2 uitings onderskeidelik, opgestel. na gelang van die data is daar onderskeidelik van linesre en paraboliese passings gebruik gemaak, waar daar van die kleinstevierkantmetode gebruik gemaak is. vir die lineere en paraboliese passings is die akkuraatheid van die passing weergegee deur die simbole r en r onderskeidelik, waar die terme telkens die fraksie van akkuraatheid voorstel (r of r = 1 stel dus 'n perfekte passing voor). 'n akkurate lineere passing impliseer dat y toeneem, namate χ toeneem. dit sou dus beteken dat antisiperingstyd toeneem na gelang konsonantstringtyd toeneem. 'n akkurate paraboliese passing impliseer dat y toeneem tot op 'n punt en daarna afneem, ten spyte van 'n toename in x. dit sou dus beteken dat antisiperingstyd 'n maksimum bereik, waarna dit nie verder toeneem nie. verwerking van data om die invloed van woordgrense op die aanvang van labiale emg-aktiwiteit te bepaal indien antisiperende labiale koartikulasie oor die woordgrens strek sal antisiperingstyd (y) groter wees as konsonantstringtyd (x) (figuur 3). 'n gepaarde t-toets die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 (eensteekproefgeval) is m.b.v. proc univariate en sas uitgevoer om te bepaal of daar 'n beduidende grootteverskil tussen die waardes van χ en y is. met behulp van die toets is 'n p-waarde verkry, en indien die waarde kleiner as 0,05 is, dui dit op 'n beduidende grootteverskil. 'n vyf persent peil van betekenis vir betekenisvolheid is dus gehandhaaf. 'n beduidende grootteverskil sou daarop dui dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie al gedurende die eerste uiting van twee uitinge voorgekom het. in hierdie geval het antisiperende labiale koartikulasie dus wel oor die woordgrens voorgekom. vervolgens is bepaal of daar 'n beduidende verskil is tussen die antisiperingstye van uitinge waarin die konsonantkombinasie /st/ binne 'n woord voorkom en vir uitinge waar die spesifieke konsonantkombinasie geskei word deur 'n woordgrens. daar moet dus bepaal word of emg-aktiwiteit telkens min of meer op dieselfde punt in die woord 'n aanvang neem, vir uitings wat as een en twee woorde uitgespreek word. die verskil tussen χ en y is bereken vir beide die uitings wat as een en twee woorde uitgespreek is. die rede waarom die verskil bereken is, is ten einde te kompenseer vir die verskil in konsonantstringtyd van die twee groepe uitings. die verkrygde waardes word dan m.b.v. 'n gepaarde ttoets (eensteekproefgeval) geevalueer ten einde te bepaal of daar 'n beduidende verskil in antisiperingstyd tussen die twee groepe uitings bestaan. met behulp van die toets is 'n p-waarde verkry en indien die waarde dus groter as 0,05 is, is die verskil nie beduidend nie. 'n vyf persent peil van betekenis vir betekenisvolheid is weer gehandhaaf. indien die verskil nie beduidend is nie, impliseer dit dat die woordgrens in hierdie konteks nie die aanvangsmoment van labiale rondingsaktiwiteit be'invloed nie. antisipering begin dus op dieselfde plek in die uitings, ongeag of die uiting as een of twee woorde uitgespreek is. karin theron, anita van der merwe en malcolm baker beskrywing van resultate bepaling van die temporale omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie die resultate van die verskillende proefpersone en segmentreekse met die geronde vokaal /u/ en !z>! word opsommender wys bespreek aangesien, daar konstant sekere tendense teenwoordig was. soos reeds genoem by die verwerking van data is daar telkens bepaal of'n linesre of paraboliese verband tussen χ en y bestaan, waar die xas telkens die duur van die konsonantstring in millisekondes aandui (konsonantstringtyd), en die y-as telkens na die tydsverloop vanaf die aanvang van emg-aktiwiteit tot die akoestiese aanset van die geronde vokaal (antisiperingstyd), ook in millisekondes, verwys. ter illustrasie van die paraboliese en lineere verband tussen χ en y word slegs figure 4 en 5 ingesluit, aangesien die resultate van die ander proefpersone deurgaans dieselfde tendens (paraboliese of lineere verband) getoon het. die roudata wat antisiperingstyd en konsonantstringtyd vir elke uiting en proefpersoon toon verskyn in bylae 1. 'n paraboliese verband tussen χ en y is twee keer verkry vir uitings van ekperimentele materiaalgroep 1 en een keer vir uitings van eksperimentele materiaalgroep 2. figuur 4 illustreer die resultate van proefpersoon 1 m.b.t. eksperimentele materiaalgroep 1 waar 'n paraboliese verband tussen χ en y bestaan. 'n paraboliese verband impliseer dat antisiperingstyd toegeneem het namate konsonantstringtyd toegeneem het, totdat laasgenoemde 'n maksimum bereik (in hierdie geval 524 ms), waarna antisiperingstyd weer afgeneem het. 'n lineere verband tussen χ en y is verkry in twee gevalle van eksperimentele materiaal groep 1 uitings en in drie gevalle van eksperimentele materiaalgroep 2 figuur 3: rekenaaruitdruk vanaf medelec mystro ms25 ter illustrasie van die voorkoms van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie oor 'n woordgrens the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) antisiperende labiale koartikulasie: 'n elektromiografiese studie by afrikaanssprekendes 43 tings. figuur 5 illustreer die resultate van proefpersoon χ1 waar 'n positiewe lineere verband tussen χ en y verkry •' vir eksperimentele materiaalgroep 2. 'n positiewe lineere v e r b a n d impliseer dat namate konsonantstringtyd (x) t o e g e n e e m het, a n t i s i p e r i n g s t y d (y) o o k t o e g e n e e m het. in twee gevalle (eksperimentele materiaalgroep 2 van proefpersoon 2 en eksperimentele materiaalgroep 1 van proefpersoon 4) kon daar nie 'n paraboliese of lineere verband tussen χ en y verkry word nie, maar met beskouing van die data is in beide die gevalle gevind dat antisiperingstyd wel toegeneem het, namate konsonantstringtyd toegeneem het tot op 'n sekere punt. a l h o e w e l d i t u i t d i e r e s u l t a t e b l y k d a t d i e a n t i s i p e r i n g s t y e v a n die v e r s k i l l e n d e p r o e f p e r s o n e vir dieselfde u i t i n g s v e r s k i l , t o o n a l d i e p r o e f p e r s o n e se resultate egter dat antisiperingstyd toeneem namate konsonantstringtyd toeneem. aangesien die verskynsel voorgekom het tydens die uitinge met die geronde vokaal lz>/ en /u/ kan afgelei word dat die twee vokale 'n ooreenstemmende effek het. antisiperingstyd het gewissel vanaf 144 ms vir die uiting /is z>/ (by proefpersoon 2) tot 950 ms vir die uiting /ikststu/ (by proefpersoon 3). al die proefpersone se antisiperingstye het vir eksperimentele materiaalgroep 2 egter 'n maksimum bereik en afgesien van 'n toename in konsonantstringtyd het antisiperingstyd na hierdie punt nie verder toegeneem nie. vir eksperimentele materiaalgroep 1 het slegs proefpersone een, twee en vyf se antisiperingstye 'n maksimum bereik. die maksimum antisiperingstye van die verskillende proefpersone het ook verskil. antlalperlngatyd (ma) 10001— 760 6 0 0 2s0 300 400 600 800 700 800 800 konsonantstringtyd (ms) antlalperlnoatyd (ma) + 800 + 800 + 800 + 800 400 + 400 200 300 400 600 800 700 konsonantstringtyd (ms) figuur 4: puntediagram van konsonantstring teenoor antisiperingstyd van eksperimentele materiaalgroep 1 vir proefpersoon 1. figuur 5: puntediagram van konsonantstringtyd teenoor antisiperingstyd van eksperimentele materiaalgroep 2 vir proefpersoon 1. tabel 3: gegewens aangaande verloop van emg-aktiwiteit in die vgk^vg-konteks. maksimum amplitude vir eerste piek in μ ν minimum amplitude tussen pieke in μ ν maksimum amplitude vir tweede piek in μ ν deurlopende emg aktiwiteit pp.1 /usu/ i 48 1 54 /uku/ 48 1 54 pp.2: /usu/ ! 60 0 75 /uku/ x pp.3: /usu/ 48,4 2 32 /uku/ x pp.4: /usu/ 40 0 44 /uku/ 27,8 1,99 22,8 pp.5: /usu/ 78 1 58 /uk 1 58 2 64 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 karin theron, anita van der merwe en malcolm baker bepaling van die verloop van labiale emgaktiwiteit in die vpjvfkonteks ter illustrasie van hierdie resultate word tabel 3 ingesluit. daar word telkens in mikrovolt aangedui wat die maksimum amplitude van die twee piekperiodes was, indien teenwoordig. verder word die minimum amplitude van die emg-aktiwiteit tussen die twee piekperiodes ook in mikrovolt aangedui. in die tabel word aangetoon dat deurlopende emg-aktiwiteit voorgekom het indien geen prominente pieke met 'n afname in emg-aktiwiteit tussen die pieke onderskei kon word tydens beoordeling deur die neuroloog en navorser nie. uit die resultate blyk dit dus dat die keepverskynsel by agt uit die tien uitings voorgekom het. daar is egter slegs van een intervokaliese konsonant gebruik gemaak en die konsonantstringtyd was dalk in die twee gevalle waar die keepverskynsel nie waargeneem is nie, korter as die tyd wat nodig is vir die spieraktiwiteit om te daal tot die basislyn vir die eerste /u/ en dan weer te styg vir die tweede /u/ (bell-berti & harris, 1982). daar word aanbeveel dat die verskynsel in die toekoms ondersoek word vir uitings waar meer as een intervokaliese konsonant gebruik .word, om te bepaal of die keepverskynsel konstant voorkom. bepaling van die effek van woordgrense op die aanvang van labiale emg-aktiwiteit tydens statistiese verwerking is die roudata van al die proefpersone vir eksperimentele materiaalgroep a en eksperimentele materiaalgroep β uitings gesamentlik verwerk. om te bepaal of antisiperende labiale koartikulasie oor woordgrense voorkom, is soos reeds genoem, gebruik gemaak van 'n gepaarde t-toets om te bepaal of die antisiperingstyd (y) groter is as konsonantstringtyd (x). indien ρ < 0,05 dan is y > x. daar is gevind dat ρ gelyk is aan 0.0001 en dus is y groter as x. die resultate impliseer dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie telkens reeds gedurende die produksie van die eerste woord voorgekom het en wel voor die produksie van die laaste segment van die eerste woord. die voorkoms van die woordgrens het dus nie die voorkoms van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie gedurende die eerste woord oor die woordgrens gei'nhibeer nie. die verskynsel het konstant by al die proefpersone voorgekom vir die sinvolle en onsinuitinge. vervolgens is bepaal of daar 'n beduidende verskil is tussen die antisiperingstye van uitinge waarin die konsonantkombinasie /st/ binne 'n woord voorkom en uitinge waar /s/en/t/ geskei word deur 'n woordgrens. weer eens is die data van al die proefpersone gesamentlik verwerk. die p-waarde wat m.b.v. die t-toets verkry is, is 0,7982. aangesien die waarde groter as 0,05 is, dui dit daarop dat daar nie 'n beduidende verskil is tussen die antisiperingstye van die uitings wat as een of twee woorde uitgespreek is nie. antisipering begin dus op ongeveer dieselfde foneem voor die geronde vokaal, ongeag of die uiting uit een of twee uitings bestaan. die resultaat impliseer dat in twee uitings soos /gistul/ en /dis/ /tul / antisipering in albei gevalle bv. by die ν sal begin. die teenwoordigheid van 'n woordgrens bei'nvloed dus nie die aanvang van antisipering nie. die verskynsel het konstant by al die proefpersone voorgekom. bespreking van resultate die temporale omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie die data van die huidige studie dui aan dat die aanvangsmoment van labiale emg-aktiwiteit (antisiperingstyd) wissel, na gelang die duur van die konsonantstring wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan wissel. daar is gevind dat liprondingsaktiwiteit toenemend vroeer 'n aanvang neem, namate die duur van die voorafgaande konsonantstring toeneem. die antisiperingstye het gewissel vanaf 144 ms (proefpersoon 4) vir die uiting usui tot 950 ms (proefpersoon 3) vir die uiting /ikststu/. alhoewel die verskillende proefpersone se antisiperingstye vir dieselfde uitings verskil het, het almal se resultate getoon dat antisiperingstyd toeneem, namate konsonantstringtyd toeneem. in sommige gevalle (proefpersone een, twee, drie en vier) het dit voorgekom of van die proefpersone hul maksimum antisiperingstyd behaal het, aangesien daar op 'n sekere punt 'n afname in antisiperingstyd was, ten spyte van 'n toename in konsonantstringtyd. die feit dat antisiperingstyd in sommige gevalle op 'n sekere punt nie verder toegeneem het nie, ten spyte van 'n toename in konsonantstringtyd, kan dui op 'n moontlike temporale beperking vir antisiperende labiale koartikulasie. in die huidige studie verskil die maksimum temporale omvang wat behaal is van persoon tot persoon. die maksimum antisiperingstyd wat 'n persoon behaal het weerspieel dus moontlik die maksimum grootte van die beplanningseenhede wat die persoon gebruik in die motoriese beplanning van sy spraak, van watter aard die eenhede ook al mag wees. die bevindings aangaande die temporale omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie kan gebruik word om sekere afleidings te maak aangaande die wyse waarop motoriese beplanning van spraak deur die brein geskied. die resultate van die huidige studie is in ooreenstemming met bevindings van ondersoekers wat meen dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie 'n wye omvang kan he (daniloff & moll, 1968; benguerel & cowan, 1974; mcallister et al., 1974). die sterk positiewe verband tussen die duur van die nie-labiale konsonantstring en die aanvangsmoment van labiale emg-aktiwiteit, ondersteun die hipotese dat die aanvang van labiale ronding beplan word in verhouding tot die temporale omvang van die nielabiale konsonantstring wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan. daar kan na aanleiding hiervan geredeneer word dat die brein soveel as bv. 950 ms (by proefpersoon 3) vooraf besef dat 'n geronde vokaal geproduseer gaan word. verder kom dit voor asof die brein ook bewus is van die feit dat daar geen aktiwiteit van die lippe vereis word in die intervokaliese periode nie, aangesien die konsonantstring nie-labiaal is. as gevolg hiervan kan vroee lipronding gei'nisieer word. die resultaat lewer dus bewys van die voorafbeplanning van spraak deur die brein en impliseer dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie 'n bespiedingsmeganisme vir spraakproduksie reflekteer (lubker & gay, 1982). hierdie bevindings is dus in teenstelling met studies waar bevind is dat die aanvang van die rondingsbeweging temporaal gebonde is aan die akoestiese aanset van die geronde vokaal en nie bei'nvloed word deur die duur van die voorafgaande konsonantstring nie (bell-berti & harris, 1979;1982; gay, 1979). 'n moontlike verklaring vir die opponerende resultate the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) isiperende labiale koartikulasie: 'n elektromiografiese studie by afrikaanssprekendes 45 van die huidige studie teenoor die van bell-berti en harris (1979;1982) kan wees dat in die spraakmateriaal wat deur bell-berti en harris (1982) gebruik is, die konsonants t r i n g t y d nooit 420 ms oorskry het nie en 'n maksimum van vier i n t e r v o k a l i e s e k o n s o n a n t e g e b r u i k is. antisiperingstyd sou dus moeilik die duur van 420 ms oorskry. verder was antisiperingstyd soms in genoemde n a v o r s e r s se studie wel 300 ms en 400 ms soos blyk uit hul studie se puntediagramme. antisiperingstyd was dus nie konstant 250 ms (bell-berti & harris, 1982) nie. die omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie kan ook gebruik word om modelle van spraakproduksie, wat poog om o.a. die v e r s k y n s e l van a n t i s i p e r e n d e koartikulasie te verklaar, te ondersoek. kozhevnikov en chistovich (in kent & minifie, 1977) postuleer dat die omvang van antisiperende koartikulasie die grootte van die eenheid wat in die beplanning van spraak gebruik word, reflekteer. na aanleiding van hulle resultate met russiese proefpersone, het hulle w a a r g e n e e m dat lipprotrusie vir konsonantkombinasies wat geronde vokale voorafgaan, begin met die eerste konsonant in die reeks. die navorsers het tot die gevolgtrekking gekom dat artikulatoriese bewegings georganiseer word in lettergrepe met die vorm van kv, kkv, kkkv ens. die lettergreep kan dus bestaan uit enige hoeveelheid konsonante, gevolg deur 'n vokaal. die basiese hipotese wat deur die kozhevnikov-chistovich teorie (in kent & minifie, 1977) gepostuleer word, is dat die motoriese beplanning van spraak onderbreek word by sekere intervalle, naamlik ηά die produksie van 'n vokaal. nadat 'n vokaal teegekom is, begin 'n nuwe beplanningseenheid. na aanleiding van hul bevindings het hulle 'n model, bekend as die artikulatoriese lettergreepmodel, gepostuleer wat poog om die verskynsel van antisiperende koartikulasie te verklaar. uit die resultate van die huidige studie blyk dit egter dat die rondingsbeweging in sommige gevalle alreeds voor of gedurende die eerste vokaal in die uiting 'n aanvang neem. die rondingsbeweging word dus alreeds voor die produksie van die eerste konsonant in die uiting gei'nisieer. die verspreiding van die rondingsbeweging tot die vokaal wat die eerste konsonant ih die reeks voorafgaan, is in ooreenstemming met die bevindings van sussman en westbury (1981), benguerel |en cowan (1974) en lubker (1981). die arikulatoriese lettergreep, bestaande uit 'n sekere hoeveelheid konsonarite wat deur 'n vokaal gevolg word, kan dus nie as die enigste basiese invoereenheid vir die motoriese beplanning van spraak aanvaar word nie. indien die omvang van antisiperende koartikulasie gebruik word as bewys van \ die grootte en aard van die beplanningseenhede van artikulasie, dan is lettergrepe in die vorm kv of selfs (k)°v (dit is enige hoeveelheid konsonante wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan) nie die enigste moontlike invoereenhede nie (kent & minifie, 1977). die a r t i k u l a t o r i e s e l e t t e r g r e e p m o d e l van kozhevnikov en chistovich (in kent & minifie, 1977) word dus nie n.a.v. die resultate van die huidige studie ondersteun nie. nog 'n model soos gepostuleer deur henke (in kent & minifie, 1977) hipotetiseer dat die invoereenheid wat vir die motoriese beplanning van spraak gebruik word, uit groepe kenmerke bestaan. henke (in kent & minifie, 1977) postuleer dat elke foneem in 'n artikulatoriese reeks uit 'n groep artikulatoriese kenmerke bestaan. elke foneem het vir 'n bepaalde kenmerk 'n waarde van nul, plus, of minus. wanneer 'n spreker 'ή artikulatoriese reeks moet produseer, voer die brein 'n bespiedingsproses uit op die foneemstring. as 'n bepaalde foneem 'n plus waarde het t.o.v. 'n spesifieke kenmerk bv. ronding, en die segmente wat dit voorafgaan almal 'n nulwaarde het vir hierdie kenmerk, sal die kenmerk tydens produksie van die tussenkomende foneme geantisipeer word. die voorafgaande foneme sal dus die kenmerkwaarde van die komende foneem aanneem. hierdie beginsel geld slegs as die kenmerk nie onverenigbaar is met die kenmerke van die tussenkomende segmente nie, en staan bekend as henke se " c o m p a t i b i l i t y n o t i o n " (kent & minifie, 1977:124). uit die huidige resultate blyk dit dat die rondingskenmerk wel tydens die produksie van die voorafgaande konsonantstring wat 'n nul waarde m.b.t. die rondingskenmerk het, geantisipeer is. die rondingskenmerk is egter in sommige gevalle reeds deur die eerste vokaal in die reeks (/i/) aangeneem. die vokaal lil het egter 'n negatiewe waarde vir die rondingskenmerk. die feit dat labiale e m g a k t i w i t e i t alreeds g e d u r e n d e die /i/ waargeneem is, is dus 'n onverwagte resultaat indien die wyse van spraakbeplanning wat deur henke (in kent & minifie, 1977) voorgestel is, aanvaar word. die bevinding dat labiale emg-aktiwiteit alreeds gedurende die /i/ voorkom, is in ooreenstemming met die bevindings van ander navorsers soos benguerel en cowan (1974), lubker (1981) en sussman en westbury (1981). aangesien die kontrole uitinge nie ronding vertoon nie, kan aanvaar word dat die resultaat nie die gevolg van 'n artefak in die meting is nie. 'n moontlike verklaring vir die resultaat is voorgestel deur sussman en westbury (1981). hierdie navorsers aanvaar ook 'n bespiedingsmeganisme wat in die motoriese beplanning van spraak deur die brein gebruik word, maar volgens hulle siening noodsaak die vokaal wat die geronde vokaal voorafgaan dat daar aanpassings gemaak moet word. indien die voorafgaande vokaal biomeganies neutraal is t.o.v. lipronding, dan behandel die bespieder die segment as nog 'n neutrale segment in die reeks. as die voorafgaande vokaal antagonisties is t.o.v. ronding dan word temporale a a n p a s s i n g s ge'inkorporeer in die a n t i s i p e r e n d e rondingsbeweging, m.a.w. ronding begin vroeer as wanneer 'n neutrale vokaal teenwoordig is. die verloop van labiale emg-aktiwiteit in 'ν v^knlvg-konteks antisiperende labiale koartikulasie blyk afwesig te wees in reekse wat gespesifiseer word as geronde vokaal (v )nie-labiale konsonant (knl)-geronde vokaal (vg). in die huidige studie is in agt van die tien uitings, twee temporaal onderskeibare pieke van musculus orbicularis oris aktiwiteit gevind, en nie oordrag van die labiale emgaktiwiteit vanaf die eerste geronde vokaal oor die konsonantstring wat neutraal is t.o.v. ronding na die tweede geronde vokaal nie (figuur 14). die bevindings stem ooreen met die bevindings van gay (1979). n a v o r s e r s wat m e e n dat a n t i s i p e r e n d e labiale koartikulasie tydsgebonde is aan die geronde vokaal meen dat die keepof dubbele piek verskynsel (lubker & gay, 1982) bewys lewer vir die werking van 'n tydsgebonde meganisme in die motoriese beplanning van spraak (gay, 1979). die n a v o r s e r s d a a r e n t e e n w a t m e e n dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie 'n wye omvang kan hs, meen egter dat die verskynsel slegs die noodsaaklikheid die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 karin theron, anita van der merwe en malcolm baker van idiosinkratiese aanpassings in beplanning, n.a.v. die aard van die eerste vokaal in die uiting, weerspieel (sussman & westbury, 1981). die keepverskynsel word ook gebruik om reedsgenoemde spraakproduksiemodelle te ondersoek. die verskynsel kan nie verklaar word as die bespiedingsmeganisme soos deur henke (in kent & minifie, 1977) gepostuleer, aanvaar word nie. volgens henke se model moes ronding ook gedurende die intervokaliese konsonant voorgekom het, aangesien die nie-labiale konsonant neutraal is t.o.v. die rondingskenmerk. dit lyk dus of die bespiedingsmeganisme in hierdie besondere geval nalaat om sy bespiedingsproses uit te voer. geen antisiperende labiale koartikulasie vind in die konteks plaas nie. die feit dat geen antisiperende koartikulasie in die konteks plaasvind nie, opponeer verder die aanname van (k)nv-eenhede as die basiese invoereenheid vir beplanning van spraak, soos gepostuleer deur kozhevnikov en chistovich (in kent & minifie, 1977), omdat volgens die navorsers labiale emg-aktiwiteit gedurende die eerste konsonant in die uiting 'n aanvang moes neem en volgehoii word oor die vokaal. dit sou wees omdat hierdie navorsers invoereenhede in die vorm van (k)nv aanvaar as die basiese invoereenheid in die motoriese beplanning van spraak. rondingsaktiwiteit het egter nie gedurende die konsonant voorgekom nie, soos blyk uit die resultate. die keepverskynsel het nie konstant voorgekom nie (in twee van die tien uitings afwesig) en is moontlik nie in genoeg diepte in die huidige studie ondersoek nie. daar is vir toetsing van die keepverskynsel gebruik gemaak van uitinge wat slegs een intervokaliese konsonant bevat in die vgkjvg-konteks. dit word aanbeveel dat die verskynsel in die toekoms in meer diepte ondersoek word deur gebruik te maak van uitinge wat langer intervokaliese konsonantstringe bevat. die effek van woordgrense op die aanvang van labiale emg-aktiwiteit in die huidige studie is gevind dat koartikulasie oor woordgrense kan voorkom. verder is gevind dat vir uitings waar 'n konsonantkombinasie /st/binne 'n woord en oor 'n woordgrens voorkom, die aanvangsmoment van ronding nie beduidend deur die woordgrens bei'nvloed word nie. die resultaat het vir beide die onsinvolle en sinvolle uitings wat as twee woorde uitgespreek is, gegeld. die resultate wat toon dat die woordgrens nie 'n beduidende effek op die aanvang van ronding het nie, is in ooreenstemming met die bevindings van daniloff en moll (1968), benguerel en cowan (1974) en bell-berti en harris (1982). die feit dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie oor 'n woordgrens kan strek ondersteun die aanname van van der merwe (1986) dat die linguisties-simboliese beplanning en die motoriese beplanning van spraak deur die brein afsonderlik geskied. die brein skei dus moontlik taal en motoriek tydens die motoriese beplanning van spraak. die onbeduidende effek van die woordgrens op die aanvang van labiale emg-aktiwiteit is ook in stryd met die aanname van 'n artikulatoriese lettergreep met die formaat (k)nv as die basiese invoereenheid wat in die motoriese beplanning van spraak gebruik word. in die huidige studie is daar in 'n uiting soos /dis/ /t mi/ gevind — u , ί.λ—μλi f x \c2 a: kursor 1 geplaas op die eerste piek van emg-aktiwiteit b: kursor 2 geplaas op die tweede peik van emg-aktiwiteit figuur 6: rekenaaruitdruk vanaf medelec mystro ms25 ter illustrasie van die keepverskynsel. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) labiale koartikulasie: ' elektromiografiese studie by afrikaanssprekendes 47 dat labiale emg-aktiwiteit al voor of gedurende die eerste ν begin en voortduur oor die woordgrens. volgens die aannames van kozhevnikov en chistovich (in kent & minifie, 1977) se artikulatoriese lettergreepmodel, behoort die l e t t e r g r e e p g r e n s die aanvang van labiale ronding ffe'inhibeer het (bell-berti & harris, 1979) en verder moes geen r o n d i n g al gedurende die /i/ voorgekom het nie. gevolgtkekking na a a n l e i d i n g van die resultate van die studie blyk dit dat nie die artikulatoriese lettergreep model van k o z h e v n i k o v en chistovich (in kent & minifie, 1977) of die bespiedingsmodel van henke (in kent & minifie, 1977) ten voile ondersteun word nie. sussman en westbury (1981) het egter 'n bespiedingsmeganisme vir die beplanning van spraak voorgestel wat deur die resultate van die huidige studie ondersteun word. hierdie navorsers meen dat 'n voldoende model van antisiperende koartikulasie 'n teoretiese beskrywing van 'n kenmerkbespiedingsmeganisme vir spraak moet bied wat aan sekere vereistes voldoen. eerstens moet die meganisme in staat wees om komende artikulatoriese kenmerke wat lank na die huidige opdragte voorkom, te antisipeer. tweedens moet die meganisme geen vaste tydsgebonde verhouding tot 'n fonetiese of abstrakte linguistiese entiteit soos 'n kenmerk of 'n foneem he nie. die feit dat gevind is dat die omvang van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie wissel na gelang konsonantstringtyd wissel, impliseer dat daar geen sodanige vaste tydsgebonde verhouding bestaan nie. derdens moet die meganisme se temporale program aangepas word om die biomeganiese vereistes van die perifere strukture, a.g.v. antagonistiese voorafgaande kontekste, te hanteer. die feit dat antisiperende labiale koartikulasie reeds gedurende die vokaal /i/ wat 'n negatiewe waarde t.o.v. ronding het voorkom, impliseer dat aanpassings gemaak word om die antagonistiese beweging van die lippe vir die /i/ te akkomodeer. vierdens moet die meganisme nie die bespiedingsvermoe gebruik wanneer 'n foneem met 'n spesifieke kenmerk bv. ronding, twee maal na mekaar voorkomjen geskei word deur 'n segment wat neutraal is t.o.v. hierdie kenmerk nie. die resultate van die huidige studie ondersteun ook hierdie aanname, aangesien antisiperende labiale koartikulasie afwesig was in die vgknlvg-|konteks. die model van sussman en westbury (19(81) verklaar egter net koartikulasie m.b.t. ronding, terwyl die beplanning van spraak alle eienskappe betrek.! dit sou dus nodig wees om ander tipes koartikulasie, bv. veler, mandibuler of linguaal, te bestudeer m.b.t. ander kenmerke bv. nasaliteit, strekking e.a. om sodoende 'n omvattende model vir die beplanning van spraak deur die brein te formuleer. die resultate van die studie ondersteun dus resultate van vele ander navorsers. uit die resultate van die huidige studie blyk dat afrikaaanssprekende persone dieselfde reageer m.b.t. die verskynsel van antisiperende labiale koartikulasie, as sprekers van ander tale. die bevinding dui daarop dat die wyse van motoriese beplanning van spraak 'n universele verskynsel is. verder dra die resultate van die studie by tot kennis aangaande antisiperende labiale koartikulasie en verskaf dit addisionele data aangaande di£ aspekte van koartikulasie waaroor daar steeds kontroversie bestaan. afleidings uit die resultate van alle navorsers m.b.t. die beplanning van spraak deur die brein is hipoteties en dit is moeilik om betroubare afleidings te maak as die geweldige kompleksiteit en wye omvang van spraakbeplanning in gedagte gehou word. verwysings bell-berti, f. & harris, k.s. (1974). more on the motor organization of speech gestures. haskins laboratories status report on speech research, sr-37/38, 73-77. bell-berti, f. & harris, k.s. (1979). anticipatory coarticulation: some implications from a study of liprounding. journal of the acoustical society of america. 65, 1268-1270. bell-berti, f. & harris, k.s. (1981). a temporal model of speech production. phonetica, 38, 9-20. bell-berti, f. & harris, k.s. (1982). temporal patterns of coarticulation: liprounding. journal of the acoustical society of america, 71, 449-454. benguerel, a.p. & cowan, h.a. (1974). coarticulation of upper lip protrusion in french. phonetica, 30, 41-55. blair, c. & smith, a. (1986). emg recording in human lip muscles: can single muscles be isolated? journal of speech and. hearing research, 29, 256-266. borden, g.j. & harris, k.s. (1984). speech science primer: physiology, acoustics and perception of speech. 2de uitgawe, baltimore: williams en wilkens. daniloff, r.g. & moll, k.l. (1968). coarticulation of liprounding. journal of speech and hearing research, 11, 707-721. gay, t. (1979). coarticulation in some consonant-vowel and consonant cluster-vowel syllables. in b. lindblom & s. oilman (reds.) frontiers of speech communication research. new york: academic. guy, r.f., edgley, c.e. arafat, i. & allen, d.e. (1987). social research methods. puzzles and solutions. allyn and bacon, inc. harris, k.s. (1984). coarticulation as a component in articulatory description. haskins laboratories: status report on speech research, sr-79, 19-36. katz, w.f. (1988a). anticipatory coarticulation in aphasia: acoustic and perceptual data. brain and language, 35, 340368. katz, w.f. (1988b). "methodological considerations" reconsidered: reply to sussman et al., brain and language, 35, 380-385. katz, w., machetanz, j., schdnle, p. & orth, u. (1990). akinematic analysis of anticipatoy coarticulation in speech of anterior aphasic subjects using electromagnetic articulography. brain and language, 38, 555-575. kent, r.d. & minifie, f.d. (1977). coarticulation in recent speech production models. journal of phonetics, 5, 115-133. lubker, j. (1981). temporal aspects of speech production: anticipatory labial coarticulation. phonetica, 38, 51-65. lubker, j. & gay, t. (1982). anticipatory labial coarticulation: experimental, biological and linguistic variables. journal of the acoustical society of america, 71, 437-448. mcallister, r., lubker, j. & carlson, j. (1974). an emg study of some characteristics of swedish rounded vowels. journal of phonetics, 2, 267-278. mclean, m. (1973). forward coarticulation of velar movement at marked junctural boundaries. journal of speech and hearing research, 16, 286-296. sussman, h.m., marquardt, t.p., mcneilage, p.f. & hutchinson, j.a. (1988). anticipatory coarticulation in aphasia: some methodological considerations. brain and language, 35,367379. sussman, h.m. & westbury, j.r. (1981). the effects of antagonistic gestures on temporal and amplitude parameters of aniticipatory labial coarticulation. journal of speech and hearing research, 22, 16-46. van der merwe, a. (1986). die motoriese beplanning van spraak by verbale apraksie. ongepubliseerde d.phil-verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 karin theron, anita van der merwe en malcolm baker bylae 1 roudata wat konsonantstringtyd en antisiperingstyd vir die onderskeie uitings van die vyf proefpersone aandui uiting konsonantstringtyd (x) (ms) antisiperingstyd (y) (ms) proefpersoon 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 /isi/ 276 192 206 154 212 184 230 306 230 246 /istu/ 292 226 192 256 300 608 420 432 266 410 /istsu/ 442 404 404 464 600 780 490 852 284 632 /iststu/ 694 586 412 578 650 790 432 874 432 562 /ikststu/ 800 668 486 604 664 116 256 950 616 544 /1s3/ 276 424 208 166 158 520 144 190 164 348 /isto/ 458 316 194 220 198 714 206 238 290 342 /istsz>/ 574 476 388 254 560 886 206 248 166 704 /iststo/ 670 524 516 502 604 854 284 718 522 508 /ikststo/ 630 640 550 520 664 780 162 688 604 296 /gistul/ 230 218 216 170 188 366 144 514 200 414 /dis/ /tula/ 350 254 318 224 320 462 182 332 302 492 /gistut/ 238 410 216 190 192 400 184 326 234 292 /dis/ /tutdrs/ 320 488 304 254 300 518 186 390 528 334 /gistup/ 308 204 228 224 226 500 222 482 358 462 /dis/ /tupa/ 326 188 332 190 282 386 206 454 222 474 /gisz>f7 236 194 236 190 216 376 238 322 336 304 /dis//tofis/ 376 190 232 160 242 504 228 592 252 412 /gis=>l/ 334 168 98 190 224 548 198 392 238 384 /dis//tola/ 340 190 863 188 230 1002 222 242 280 416 /gisz>m/ 290 208 214 206 180 406 226 316 502 ι 396 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 97 'η elektroglottograflese analise van sekere stemparameters: normatiewe aanduidings kobie cloete emily groenewald anita van der merwe departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria jean badenhorst departement statistiek universiteit van pretoria opsomming 'n omvattende stemevaluasie behels meer as net die evaluasie van perseptuele en organiese aspekte. vir 'n volledige stemevaluasie is dit belangrik dat verskeie objektiewe metings die diagnostiese battery aanvul. ten einde normatiewe riglyne vir sekere kwantitatiewe elektro-glottografiese metings vir die universiteit van pretoria se spraaknavorsingslaboratorium daar te stel, is 25 mans en 25 dames metperseptueel normale stemme, uit 'n ouderdomspektrum van 8 tot 80 jaar, ondersoek met betrekking tot gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie, piek-tot-piek frekwensiefluktuasie, piektot-piek amplitudefluktuasie en harmoniek /ruis-verhouding. resultate dui aan dat al hierdie parameters sensitiefis vir verandering in stemproduksie as gevolg van veroudering, vokaalen luidheidsveranderinge. die waarde van die verkree data vir navorsing en die kliniese praktyk word bespreek. abstract a comprehensive voice evaluation comprises more than just the evaluation of perceptual and organic aspects. objective voice analysis should supplement the diagnostic battery. in order to obtain normative indications for the speech research laboratory at the\university of pretoria, 25 men and 25 women ranging in age from 8 to 80 years and with perceptually normal voices were tested with regard to fundamental frequency, cycle to cycle frequency fluctuation (jitter), cycle to cycle amplitude fluctuation (shimmer) and harmonic i noise ratio. results indicate that these parameters are sensitive to changes in voice production caused by ageing, changes in loudness and the production of different vowels. the implications of these results for research and clinical practice are discussed. 'n omvattende stemevaluasie behels meer as net die evaluasie van perseptuele en organiese aspekte. vir 'n volledige stemevaluasie is' dit belangrik dat verskeie objektiewe metings die diagnostiese battery aanvul (gould, 1988; glaze, bless & susser, 1990). stemlaboratoria, bestaande uit onder andere verskeie akoestiese meetinstrumente, het die afgelope dekade vinnig toegeneem (gould, 1988). die ideale stemlaboratorium verskaf optimale informasie vir diagnose en behandeling van 'n stempasient en kan gebruik word vir verdere navorsing om steeds meer gesofistikeerde pasientsorg te verseker (sataloff, spiegel, carroll, darby, hawkshaw & rulnick, 1990). die spraaknavorsingslaboratorium van die universiteit van pretoria beskik oor die nodige instrumentasie vir die uitvoer van elektroglottograflese (egg) metings, maar weens 'n gebrek aan normatiewe data is dit moeilik om hierdie tegniek sinvol aan te wend. hierdie behoefte aan norme blyk ook uit die literatuur. (wilcox & horii, 1980; hollien, 1987; colton & conture, 1990; fitch, 1990; glaze etal., 1990; sataloff et al., 1990). hierdie studie beoog dus die uitbreiding van stemanalisemetodiek deur die daarstelling van normatiewe riglyne vir egg-metings, vir gebruik met normale en patologiese stemme. die elektroglottograaf is 'n objektiewe, nie-indringende meetinstrument wat gebruik word om stembandaktiwiteit te ondersoek sonder veel ongemak of inmenging met fonasie (gould, 1988). dit meet die elektriese stroom tussen twee elektrodes aan weerskante van die larinks soos wat dit deur die nekweefsel en stembande beweeg (mcfarlane & watterson, 1991). die resultaat is 'n weerstandsgolfpatroon. normale stembandvibrasie produseer 'n reeks relatief eweredige weerstandsgolwe en word as 'n gelykmatige klank gehoor, ten spyte van minimale fluktuasie in die golfpatroon (mcfarlane & watterson, 1991). die s t e m b a n d e is 2 o n a f h a n k l i k e v i b r a t o r s wat elk funksioneer op grond van die inherente fisiologiese eienskappe (bv. stembandmassa, -spanning, spieraktiwiteit, neurale aktiwiteit en kardio-vaskulere die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 98 kobie cloete, emil betrokkenheid) daarvan. indien die gekombineerde werking van die twee afsonderlike stembande 'n reeks weerstandsveranderinge tot gevolg het wat varieer in terme van tyd en/of amplitude, word die stem as grof, skor of hees waargeneem (horii, 1980; hollien, 1987; orlikoff & kahane, 1991). akoestiese metings, naamlik piek-tot-piek frekwensiefluktuasie ("jitter") en piek-tot-piek amplitudefluktuasie ("shimmer") reflekteer onderskeidelik die stabiliteit en reelmaat van stembandvibrasie. lae piektot-piek frekwensiefluktuasie (ppff)-waardes dui dus op relatief hoe stabiliteit in die frekwensie van stembandvibrasie en lae piek-tot-piek amplitudefluktuasie (ppaf)-waardes dui op 'n hoe mate van reelmaat in die amplitude van stembandvibrasie (orlikoff en kahane, 1991). uit die literatuur blyk dit dat ppff en ppaf baie hoer waardes by growwe, skor, en hees stemme het. piek-tot-piek fluktuasies is ook baie groter in patologiese stemme as in normale stemme (horii, 1980; hollien, 1987; brown, morris & michel, 1989; linville, korabic & rosera, 1990). onlangs is 'n kwantitatiewe stemanalise-program vir die kay dspsonagraph (model 5500) beskikbaar gestel. hierdie program bepaal, m.b.v. 'n elektroglottograaf, outomaties gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie (gem. fo), ppff, ppaf en gee 'n harmoniek/ruis-verhouding (wat 'n aanduiding gee van die hoeveelheid ruis of heesheid in die stem). navorsing dui daarop dat 'n akoestiese analise van die stem moontlik die eerste akkurate informasie aangaande patologiese verandering in die larinks verskaf (baken, 1987). veroudering impliseer nie noodwendig 'n patologiese toestand nie, maar normale anatomiese en fisiologiese verandering tree wel met veroudering in (hollien, 1987). veranderinge in perseptuele en akoestiese eienskappe in die verouderende stem kan aan ouderdomsverwante anatomiese en fisiologiese veranderinge op molekulere, sellulere en orgaanvlak toegeskryf word, wat dwarsdeur die hele liggaam plaasvind (aronson, 1985; chodzko-zajko & ringel, 1987; biever & bless, 1989; orlikoff, 1990a). volgens orlikoff (1990a) word die fonatoriese sisteem die meeste van enige komponent in die spraakmeganisme deur veroudering geaffekteer. dit blyk dus dat 'n behoefte aan bruikbare norme vir verskillende ouderdomsgroepe bestaan. metode doelstellings die doel van die studie is die uitvoering van 'n egganalise van spesifieke stemparameters van sprekers met perseptueel normale stemme, ten einde normatiewe riglyne daar te stel. die subdoelstellings is om die invloed van ouderdom asook vokaalen luidheidsveranderinge op die volgende stemparameters na te gaan: gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie (gem. fo) by manlike en vroulike sprekers piek-tot-piek frekwensiefluktuasie (ppff) piek-tot-piek amplitudefluktuasie (ppaf) harmoniek/ruis-verhouding (h/r-verhouding) groenewald, anita van der merwe, jean badenhorst navorsingsontwerp hierdie studie behels 'n kruisseksie-opname (guy, edgley, arafat & allen, 1987) deur middel van die korrelatiewe navorsingstegniek (smit, 1983). 'n kwotasteekproef word gebruik om proefpersone te selekteer. proefpersone word sodoende in verskillende kombinasies van ouderdom en geslag georden, om 'n objektiewe beeld van die stemeienskappe van persone van verskillende geslagte, in spesifieke ouderdomsgroepe, langs 'n ouderdomskontinuum te verkry. hierdie tipe navorsingsontwerp het dan ook die voordeel dat veranderlikes onder natuurlike omstandighede bestudeer kan word en die ko-variasie tussen die onderskeie veranderlikes ondersoek kan word. informasie wat deur die opname-studie verkry word, kan na die hele populasie veralgemeen word (guy et al., 1987). hierdie tipe studie is dus voordelig om te gebruik vir die opstel van 'n normbasis. proefpersone kriteria vir die aeleksie van proefpersone: ouderdom: aangesien die beplande databasis vir 'n ouderdomspektrum opgestel word, is proefpersone binne die volgende vyf oudersomsintervalle ondersoek: 8 tot 10 jaar die kinders moet geen tekens van puberteit toon nie (glaze et al., 1990), aangesien spesifiek 'n prepuberteitsanalise van die stem gemaak word. 19 tot 23 jaar -hierdie post-puberteitsanalise van die stem behoort optimale stembandf u n k s i o n e r i n g te reflekteer, aangesien maksimum effektiwiteit in liggaamlike funksionering origeveer in hierdie tydperk plaasvind (orlikoff, 1990a). ; 30 tot 40 j a a r o r l i k o f f ( 1 9 9 0 a ) p o s t u l e e r dat liggaamlike funksionering toenemend vinniger begin afneem vanaf ongeveer dertigjarige ouderdom. deur hierdie groep in te sluit kan die aanvang van stemveroudering gemonitor word. 50 tot 60 jaar-hierdie kategorie word ingesluit om die status van stembandfunksionering te e v a l u e e r net voor w e r k l i k e veroudering begin intree. 70 tot 80 jaar:-na die sesde lewensdekade is dit moeilik om groepsgemiddelde op^persone toe te pas, selfs al vind normale veroudering plaas (orlikoff, 1990b). as gevolg van baie kontroversie in die literatuur (shipp & hollien, 1969; horii & ryan, 1981; hollien, 1987), blyk dit nodig te wees om aparte norme, spesifiek vir hierdie ouderdoms-groep, daar te stel. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η elektroglottografiese analise van sekere stemparameters 99 ander faktore: 5 manlike en 5 vroulike proefpersone per ouderdomsgroep is geselekteer op grond van perseptueel normale stem. geen relevante mediese probleme mag teenwoordig wees nie. persone mag nie meer as 20 sigarette per dag rook nie, moet oor normale gehoor beskik, nie oormatig oorgewig wees nie en nie medikasie gebruik wat 'n uitdrogingseffek op die larinks het nie (orlikoff, 1990a; hollien, 1987; horii, 1980; fitch, 1990; baken 1987). materiaal die vokale /a:/ en hj word geselekteer vir hierdie studie. uit die literatuur blyk dit dat hierdie twee vokale die meeste vir egg-opnames gebruik word (childers & lee, 1991). die vokale /a:/ en /i:/ verteenwoordig die grootste moontlike fisiologiese omvang vir vokaalproduksie (fitch, 1990). aangesien persone met s t e m p a t o l o g i e d i k w e l s p r o b l e m e o n d e r v i n d met luidheidsveranderings, word elke vokaal ook met verhoogde luidheid geproduseer sodat 'n basis verkry kan word waarmee patologiese luidheidverandering vergelyk kan word. apparaat die volgende apparaat is gebruik vir die stemopnames: "portable electro-laryngograph" met elektrodes, electro-voice mikrofoon model 63ib, kay dsp sonagraph model 5500 en nec multisync ii vertoonskerm. die opstelling van die apparaat is gedoen soos voorgestel in die handleiding van die sonagraaf (kay elemetrics corp., 1989). die analise is met behulp van die outomatiese stemanaliseprogram, model 5625, van die dsp sonagraph gedoen. prosedure vir dataversameling die plasing van die elektrodes op die proefpersoon se larinks het volgens die prosedure, soos aanbeveel deur'mcfarlane en watterson (1991) en colton en conture (1990), geskied. optimale elektrodeplasing en opname-instellings is telkens getoets deur voorafgaande proefopnames (kay elemetrics corp., 1989). die vokaal /a/ en lil is op dieselfde wyse geproduseer, opgeneem en ontleed. elke vokaal is vir 2 sekondes geproduseer by normale gesprekstoonhoogte en -luidheid, gevolg deur 'n produksie van 2 sekondes teen verhoogde luidheid. elke uiting is 3 maal herhaal en afsonderlik ontleed, om sodoende die invloed van intrapersoonlike variasie te bekamp. prosedure vir data-analise die analiseprosedure by elke uiting, het bestaan uit die afbakening van ongeveer 100 siklusse van 'n stabiele, middelgedeelte van die lx-sein, soos op die skerm geposisioneer. (horii, 1980; linville et'al., 1990). die onderskeie parameters is dan outomaties bereken deur die aktivering van die stemanaliseprogram. die meting van gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie fundamentele frekwensie word bepaal deur die vibrasiespoed van die stembande en bepaal die toonhoogte van fonasie. die gemiddelde fo is telkens bereken vir 'n periode van ongeveer 100 siklusse. die meting van ppfv lieberman (1963) definieer ppff ("jitter") as die s i k l u s t o t s i k l u s fluktuasie in fundamentele frekwensie. die stemanaliseprogram gebruik 'n manipuleerde vorm van koike se formule (kay elemetrics corp., 1989) om ppff te bereken. resultate word as persentasie (%) weergegee. die meting van ppaf ppaf ("shimmer") of soos horii (1980) dit definieer, siklus-tot-siklus amplitudefluktuasie, word in hierdie studie bepaal deur die verskil tussen die mees positiewe en mees negatiewe pieke te bereken (kay elemetrics corp., 1989). die verkree ppaf-waardes word in desibel (db) uitgedruk. die meting van harmoniek/ruis-verhouding die h/r-verhouding druk die verskil in db uit tussen die harmoniese en ruiskomponente in die afgebakende deel van die stemgolf. dus, hoe kleiner die v e r s k i l w a a r d e tussen die a m p l i t u d e van die harmoniese komponent en die amplitude van die ruis* komponent, hoe kleiner is die h/r-verhoudingswaarde. 'n klein h/r-verhouding dui op 'n hoe mate van ruis in die stem en word as heesheid gehoor (yumoto, 1988). prosedure van dataverwerking 'n variansie analise is op die finale data gedoen met behulp van proc anova en die sas-programpakket. hierdie analise het getoets vir enige betekenisvolle verskille tussen die verskeie veranderlikes. die peil van betekenis was bepaal by p<0.05. uit die data is statistiese gegewens aangaande die verskeie veranderlikes verkry om sodoende frekwensieverdelings, gemiddeldes, standaardafwykings, minimumen maksimumwaardes te bereken. uit die finale data is vertrouensintervalle ook opgestel vir verskeie veranderlikes. 'n 95% vertrouensinterval is gekonstrueer sodat met 95% sekerheid gese kan word dat hierdie interval wel die populasie-gemiddeld sal bevat (waar populasie beduie op die totale groep van persone waarop hierdie studie betrekking het). hierdie interval is gebruik om 'n skatting te maak van die onbekende populasiegemiddeld (steyn, smit & du toit, 1987; bowermann & o'connel, 1990). resultate en bespreking die resultate word beskryf en bespreek aan die hand van die onderskeie subdoelstellings. gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie ouderdomsverwante verandering in gemiddelde fundamentele frekwensie van mans en vroue. die gem. fo van die mansen vrouegroep verskil statisties betekenisvol op 'n peil van 5%. die resultate vir die 2 groepe word dus afsonderlik verskaf. in figuur 1 word die gem. fo van mans in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe weergegee vir produksie van /a:/ en hj met normale en verhoogde luidheid. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 100 kobie cloete, emily groenewald, anita van der merwe, jean badenhorst gem. fo (hz) 350 η 300 250 2 0 0 150 1 0 0 50 0 1 8-10 i 19-23 i 30-40 i 50-60 i 70-80 /ay normaal 268.4 125.1 119.6 121.4 150.2 /ay hard 284.4 133 134 137.2 166.2 irj normaal 271.3 136.4 137.8 145.8 182.4 lij hard 294.1 146.4 153.6 166.4 196.1 ouderdomsgroepe flare) fonasie /a:/ normaal μ hard ""·"" fi:l normaal —θ μ hard figuur 1: die verandering in gem. fo van mans in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe tydens produksie van /a:/ en /i:/ met normale en verhoogde luidheid. 350 300 250 200 gem. fo (hz) 350 300 250 200 » v v ν λ ο 350 300 250 200 κ 150 100 50 0 0 1 i i 1 8-10 19-23 30-40 50-60 70-80 /a:/ normaal 291.5 250.1 221.1 233.3 200.6 /ay hard 305.8 253.9 226.2 239.7 216.6 /i:/ normaal 299.8 261.4 233.2 251.1 244.1 /i:/ hard 331.3 267.3 239.9 261.8 267.4 ouderdomsgroepe flare) fonasie 1 /a:/ normaal ·• /&/ hard 1 fr.l normaal μ hard figuur 2: die verandering in gem. fo van vroue in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe tydens produksie van die vokale /a:/ en /i:/ met normale en verhoogde luidheid. daar is 'n skerp daling in gem. fo na puberteit. slegs die 8-10 jaar groep verskil statisties, op 'n 5% peil van betekenis, van die ander ouderdomsgroepe. die verlaging in fo na puberteit, is die logiese gevolg van fisiese groei van die fonasiestruktuur. dit word gevolg deur 'n neiging tot verhoging in gem. fo met veroudering, wat moontlik toegeskryf kan word aan ouderdomsverwante veranderinge wat in die larinks plaasvind, bv. verdunning van die stembande a.g.v. atrofie, dehidrasie van die laringale m u k o s a , v e r m i n d e r i n g in die elastisiteit van die laringale ligamente, toenemende ossifikasie en kalsifikasie van die k r a a k b e n i g e strukture van die larinks en afname in sentrale senuweestelselkontrole (aronson, 1985; chodzko-zajko & ringel, 1987; orlikoff, 1990b). die gem. fo waardes verkry in die huidige studie korreleer met die resultate van ander navorsers (aronson, 1985; colton & casper, 1990; childers & lee, 1991). in figuur 2 word die gem. fo van vroue in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe weergegee tydens produksie van die vokale /a:/ en /i:/ met normale en verhoogde luidheid. figuur 2 vertoon 'n geleidelike verlaging in gem. fo van die vokale /a:/ en /i:/ vanaf die ouderdomsgroep 810 jaar tot by die ouderdomsgroep 30-40 jaar. hierdie verlaging in die gem. fo van vroue is nie so groot as die verlaging in gem. fo wat by mans in dieselfde tydperk voorkom nie. die laagste gemiddelde fo-waardes word deur die groep 70-80 jaar vertoon. die gemiddelde fowaardes van die verskillende ouderdomsgroepe verskil egter nie statisties, op 'n 5% peil van betekenis, nie. dit kom voor asof die invloed van veroudering nie e e n v o r m i g e gem. fo v e r a n d e r i n g e by alle vroue teweegbring nie, terwyl 'n styging in gem. fo by feitlik alle mans voorkom. uit die literatuur blyk baie kontroversie rondom die invloed van veroudering op die gem. fo van vroue (mcglone & hollien, 1963) en sommige navorsers (aronson, 1985), voel dat hierdie verskillende neigings nie verklaar kan word nie. hollien (1987) postuleer egter dat menopouse die omgekeerde van puberteit is. die neiging van mans se gem. fo om na die ouderdom van 50-60 jaar toenemend te begin styg en vroue se gem. fo wat neig om te daal, staan in1 die literatuur bekend as die "sentrale neiging" (orlikoff, 1990b) wat hollien (1987) se siening dat die menopciuse die omgekeerde effek as puberteit op gem. fo (het bevestig. j vokaalverwante verandering in gem. fo ^an mans en vroue ( soos dit blyk uit figure 1 en 2 is die gem. fo van /i:/ telkens hoer as vir /a:/. die /i:/ is 'n hoe, voorvokaal en produksie van hierdie vokaal behels dat die tong gelig word. met die oplig van die tong word die hioi'edbeen en larinks opwaarts getrek en die laringale spiere word gestrek. gevolglik verhoog die elastisiteit van die stembande en het hoer gem. fo waardes vir ivj tot gevolg (aronson, 1985). . ^ luidheidsverwante veranderinge in gem. fo van mans en vroue fonasie met verhoogde luidheid het telkens hoer gem. fo-waardes as normale fonasie tot gevolg. hierdie verhoging in gem. fo tydens harde fonasie kan gesien the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η elektroglottografiese analise van sekere stemparameters 101 word as die resultaat van veranderinge in o.a. subglottale lugdruk wat vir klankpeilvariasie benodig word. tydens harde fonasie kom die grootste gem. foverhoging by die 70-80 jaar voor. verskeie outeurs rapporteer 'n toename in die veranderlikheid van gem. fo van bejaardes wat toegeskryf kan word aan 'n verlies aan aanpasbaarheid as gevolg van 'n vermindering in spiertonus en -krag, ossifikasie en ander ouderdomsverwante veranderinge in die bindweefsel van die laringale kraakbeen. (hollien, 1987; colton & casper, 1990; orlikoff, 1990a; biever & bless, 1989; brown et al., 1989). piek-tot-piek frekwensiefl uktuasie statistics betekenisvolle verskille kom nie by die piek-tot-piek frekwensiefluktuasie (ppff) ("jitter")waardes van mans en vroue voor nie. om hierdie rede word die verkree resultate gekombineer en weergegee in figuur 3. 1 ppff (%) 0.7 0.6 0.5 / * 0.4 // ϊ / // 0.3 0.2 i\\ .y y 0.1 0 i i 8-10 19-23 30-40 50-60 70-80 /aj normaal 0.357 0.241 0.334 0.344 0.678 /av/hard 0.308 0.192 0.166 0.307 0.464 ivj normaal 0.334 0.20j1 0.235 0.344 0.542 lij hard 0.285 0.219 0.159 0.287 0.449 \ ouderdomsgroepe (jare) ! —— lay normaal ftu/ hard —*— /i:/normaal; b μ hard figuur 3:ppff-waardes van mans en dames in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe tydens produksie van /a:/ en /i:/ met normale en verhoogde luidheid. ouderdomsverwante verandering in ppff lae ppff-waardes reflekteer relatief hoe stabiliteit in stembandvibrasie. die groep 19-23 jaar verkry dan ook die laagste gemiddelde ppff-waarde vir /a:/ en /i:/, wat dui op die mees stabiele stembandvibrasie van al die ouderdomsgroepe. orlikoff (1990a) rapporteer dat liggaamsfunksionering maksimum effektiwiteit tussen 20-29 jarige ouderdom bereik. die lae ppff-waardes van die groep 19-23 jaar korrelleer dan ook met hierdie stelling van orlikoff. teen dertigjare ouderdom begin homeostatiese funksionering, sowel as kompensatoriese en regulatoriese kontrole in die liggaam afneem. ppff bereik dan ook 'n maksimum gemiddelde waarde (figuur 3) by die groep 70-80 j a a r en v e r s k i l s t a t i s t i c s betekenisvol, op 'n 5% peil van betekenis, van die ander ouderdomsgroepe. hierdie hoer ppff-waardes wat korreleer met 'n toename in ouderdom dui dus op 'n geleidelike verlies in stabiliteit in stembandvibrasie en word geassosieer met growwe, skor en hees stemme (hollien, 1987; horii, 1980; orlikoff, 1990a). alle bejaardes vertoon nie hoe ppff-waardes nie. hierdie verskynsel word in die literatuur verklaar aan die hand van die fisiese gesondheid van die proefpersone, veral wat betref kardiovaskulere gesondheid (biever & bless, 1989; brown et al., 1989; orlikoff, 1990b). orlikoff (1990a) vind dat gesonde, bejaarde mans se ppff ooreenstem met die ppff van mans in hul twintigerjare, terwyl bejaarde mans met aterosklerose baie hoer ppff-waardes as die gesonde jong en bejaarde mans vertoon. dit is dus duidelik dat sekere gesondheidsverwante faktore ppff-resultate kan bei'nvloed. dit/is verder ook bekend dat alle liggaamlike sisteme nie ewe vinnig verouder nie en dat laringale veroudering by sommige persone vinniger as by ander kan plaasvind (chodzko-zajko & ringel, 1987). by die opstel van normatiewe riglyne moet die wye omvang van ppff-waardes van bejaardes dus ingedagte gehou word en kan groepsgemiddelde nie sonder meer op die geriatriese populasie toegepas word nie. vokaalverwante verandering in ppff die verskillende vokale het nie 'n statisties betekenisvolle invloed op ppff nie, alhoewel dit voorkom asof /a:/ meer konstante resultate oplewer. hierdie resultate korreleer ook met die van wilcox & horii (1980). luidheidsverwante veranderinge in ppff uit die resultate van ppff blyk dit dat fonasie met verhoogde luidheid feitlik konstant laer waardes oplewer as fonasie met normale luidheid. glaze et al. (1990) postuleer dat luidheidsveranderinge teweeggebring word deur veranderinge in subglottale lugdruk en die mediale kompressie van die stembande wat gevolglik die stembandvibrasiepatroon verander deur verlenging van die geslote fase van stembandvibrasie. die v e r a n d e r i n g e wat in die s p r a a k p r o d u k s i e meganisme plaasvind vir harde fonasie kan dus moontlik bydra tot groter stabiliteit in die vibrasiepatroon van die stembande en lewer gevolglik laer ppff-waardes op. piek-tot-piek amplitudefl uktuasie geen statisties betekenisvolle verskille kom by die piek-tot-piek amplitudefluktuasie (ppaf) ("shimmer")waardes van die twee geslagte voor nie en daarom word mans en dames se resultate gekombineerd in figuur 4 weergegee. figuur 4 stel die verandering van ppaf met toename in ouderdom voor, vir die vokale /a:/ en /i:/ tydens produksie met normale en verhoogde luidheid. ouderdomsverwante veranderinge in ppaf die ppaf-waardes van die verskillende ouderdomsgroepe verskil nie statisties, op 'n 5% peil van betekenis, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 102 kobie cloete, emily groenewald, anita van der merwe, jean badenhorst p p a f (db) 0.5 0.4 0.3 \ \ ' , 0.3 \ ' , 0.3 -g 0.2 * ' 0.2 ' • * * ' 0.1 0 ι — i 8-10 19-23 30-40 50-60 70-80 /a:/ normaal 0.288 0.232 0.353 0.343 0.444 laj hard 0.251 0.189 0.212 0.314 0.329 lij normaal 0.359 0.307 0.42 0.33 0.343 ivj hard 0.346 0.268 0.314 0.249 0.26 ouderdomsgroepe flare) ——/aynormaal ·•*•• /&:/ hard —»— flu normaal e μ hard figuur 4: ppaf-waardes van mans en dames in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe tydens produksie van l&'j en /is/ met normale en verhoogde luidheid. van mekaar nie. lae ppaf-waardes reflekteer 'n hoe mate van reelmaat in stembandvibrasie. die groep 1923 jaar behaal vir beide vokale /a:/ en iv.l die laagste ppaf-waardes wat dui op die mees reelmatige stembandvibrasie van al die ouderdomsgroepe. hierdie resultate korreleer met o r l i k o f f (1990a) se stelling dat maksimum effektiwiteit in liggaamsfunksionering ongeveer in die tydperk 20-29 jaar plaasvind. glaze et al. (1990) vind dat ppaf-waardes vir kinders 5-11 jaar buite die ppaf-waardes van volwassenes le. die groep 8-10 jaar van die huidige studie, vertoon ook hoer waardes by k i n d e r s op grond van voortdurende anatomiese en morfologiese struktuurveranderinge op hierdie ouderdom. tydens die maak van opnames vir die huidige studie is ook gevind dat opnames oor die algemeen moeiliker vir kinders as volwassenes gemaak word, a.g.v. moeiliker elektrodeplasing. 'n swak golfpatroon is ook in sommige gevalle verkry, wat die gevolg kan wees van meer vetweefsel in die nek wat algemeen by kinders voorkom (colton & conture, 1990; baken, 1987), maar ook die gevolg van swak elektrodekontak op die klein en hooggeplaaste larinks. hierdie veranderlikes dra moontlik by tot die hoer ppafen ppff-waardes wat vir kinders in die huidige, sowel as ander studies verkry is (colton & conture, 1990; glaze et al., 1990). net soos die geval by ppff, korreleer die hoer gemiddelde ppaf-waardes met 'n toename in ouderdom en dui op 'n geleidelike verlies aan reelmaat in stembandvibrasie soos wat 'n persoon verouder. hoe ppafen ppff-waardes is direkte korrelate van 'n growwe, skor en hees stem, en kan verwag word dat die verouderende stem toenemend grof, skor en hees sal klink. dit is egter nie altyd die geval nie. tydens die huidige studie behaal sommige bejaardes ppaf-waardes wat korreleer met die waardes van die groep 19-23 jaar. net soos die geval by ppff kan die afleiding gemaak word dat fisiese gesondheid met stemfunksie verband hou. alhoewel die proefpersone van die huidige studie gesond was, is faktore soos arteriosklerose en osteop o r o s e nie spesifiek g e k o n t r o l e e r nie. h i e r d i e veranderlikes, wat nie in dieselfde mate by die bejaarde proefpersone voorkom nie, kon ook moontlik bydra tot hoer ppaf(en ppff)-waardes (chodzko-zajko & ringel, 1987; brown et al., 1989; orlikoff, 1990b). vokaalverwante veranderinge in ppaf by vergelyking van ppaf-resultate van /a:/ en ivj blyk dit dat die vokaal /a:/ beter is om te gebruik vir die meting van ppaf as ivj, aangesien dit voorkom asof ivj meer onkonstante resultate oplewer as /a:/. hier kan onkonstante ppaf-resultate van ivj moontlik toegeskryf word aan die opwaartse verplasing van die larinks tydens produksie van ivj, aangesien die elektrodes spesifiek geplaas word vir produksie van die vokaal /a:/ en nie vir /i:/-produksie aangepas word nie. luidheidsverwante veranderinge in ppaf figuur 4 toon aan dat fonasie met verhoogde luidheid telkens laer ppaf-waardes as normale fonasie tot gevolg het. dit wil voorkom asof die veranderinge wat in die stemproduksie-meganisme vir fonasie met verhoogde luidheid plaasvind, bydra tot groter reelmaat in stembandvibrasie en gevolglik laer ppaf-waardes oplewer as normale fonasie. dit is dus duidelik uit huidige resultate dat stemluidheid gekontroleer moet word tydens metings. harmoniek/ruis-verhouding die h/r-verhoudingswaardes (h/r-verhouding) van mans en vroue verskil nie statisties betekenisvol, pp 'n peil van 5% betekenis, van mekaar nie en daarom is die resultate gekombineer en weergegee in figuur 5. figuur 5 stel die v e r a n d e r i n g in h / r v e r h o u d i n g met veroudering vir die vokale /a:/ en ivj tydens produksie met normale en verhoogde luidheid voor. | ι ouderdomsverwante veranderinge in h/rverhouding i in figuur 5 is dit duidelik dat h/r-verhoudiiigswaardes 'n dalende tendens met veroudering vertoon. die h/r-verhoudingswaardes verskil nie statisties, op 'n 5% peil van betekenis, vir die groepe 8-10, 19-23, 3040 en 50-60 jaar nie. die h/r-waardes van die 70-80 jaar groep, vir fonasie teen normale luidheid, verskil wel statisties betekenisvol van die ander groepe. in teenstelling met ppff en ppaf reflekteer 'n hoer h/rverhouding beter stembandfunksionering. volgens yumoto (1988) is die h/r-verhouding 'n goeie aanduiding van die graad van heesheid wat in die stem voorkom, aangesien die h/r-verhouding aandui in hoe 'n mate die harmoniese komponent deur die ruiskomponent in die stem verplaas word. uit figuur 5 wil dit dus voorkom asof ouderdomsverwante involusie van die laringale strukture heesheid tot gevolg kan he, en moontlik tot the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η elektroglottografiese analise van sekere stemparameters 103 25 22.5 20 17.5 15 12.5 10 h/r verhouding (db) 8-10 19-23 30-40 50-60 70-80 laj normaal laj hard ivj normaal ivj hard 17.9 19.5 17.3 18.2 17.5 18.5 17.8 16.6 17.6 18 18.4 18.5 15.4 15.4 17.8 17.9 13 14.3 14.8 15.5 ouderdomsgroepe flare) /a:/ normaal /i:/ normaal •·*·• /&:/ hard μ hard figuur 5: die h/r-verhoudingswaarde van persone in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe tydens produksie van la.il en liil met normale en verhoogde luidheid. perseptuele verskille tussen ou en jong stemme bydra (shipp & hollien, 1969; hollien, 1987). alle bejaarde persone behaal egter nie lae h/r-verhoudingswaardes in die huidige studie nie. soos in die geval by ppff en ppaf kan die afleiding gemaak word dat alle persone se laringale strukture nie diieselfde mate van involusie met veroudering ondergaan nie (chodzko-zajko & ringel, 1987), en dat heesheid eerder met patologie van die larinks as met veroudering verband hou. vokaalverwante verandering in h/r-verhouding ι soos dit blyk uit die resultate van ppaf, ppff en ook h / r v e r h o u d i n g , lewer die vokaal /a:/ meer konstante resultate op as iv.l. die meeste studies wat h/r-verhouding ondersoek, gebruik dan ook die vokaal /a:/ vir opnames (yumoto, 1988). luidheidsverwante verandering in h/r-verhouding uit figuur 5 blyk dit dat hoer gemiddelde h/rverhoudingswaardes telkens vir harde fonasie as vir normale fonasie behaal word. dit blyk dus 'dat harde fonasie telkens beter stemband-funksionering tot gevolg het as normale fonasie. aanduidings vir die daarstelling van 'n normatiewe basis die resultate van die gemiddelde fo-waardes vir mans en vrouens word o.g.v. die betekenisvolle statistiese verskille tussen die 2 groepe, afsonderlik beskou. hoewel slegs die 8-10-jaar-groep van die mans statisties, op 'n 5% peil van betekenis, van die ander ouderdomsgroepe verskil, word die gegewens vir die verskillende ouderdomsgroepe nogtans afsonderlik verskaf. dit word gedoen na aanleiding van vorige navorsingsbevinding wat daarop dui dat luisteraars perseptuele verskille tussen ouderdomsgroepe waarneem. (horii & ryan, 1981; biever & bless, 1989; brown et al., 1989). gegewens van gemiddelde fo-waardes, word verskaf in bylaag 1. 'n statistiese berekening van 95% vertrouensintervalle is nie vir die gem. fo-waardes gedoen nie, omdat die data vir mans en vroue nie gekombineer kon word nie en die groepe gevolglik te klein was. 'n normatiewe basis vir verskillende ouderdomsgroepe betreffende ppff-, ppafen h/r-verhoudingswaardes word verskaf in bylae 2, 3 en 4 respektiewelik. die resultate van mans en vroue kan gekombineer word omdat geen betekenisvolle verskil bestaan nie. die gegewens vir elke parameter is verkry deur statistiese berekenings van vertrouensintervalle, wat geld vir 95% van die populasie. aangesien die produksie van /a:/ deurgaans meer konstante en betroubare resultate gelewer het, is die waardes vir 'n normatiewe basis geselekteer. die verkree resultate van ppff-, ppafen h/r-verhoudingswaardes korreleer met die van ander navorsers en kan dus as betroubaar aanvaar word. (horii, 1980; kay elemetrics corp., 1989; brown etal., 1989; wilcox & horii, 1980; orlikoff en kahane, 1991; glaze et al., 1990). gevolgtrekkings uit die resultate van die huidige studie kan die volgende afleidings gemaak word: gem. fo-waardes van mans en vrouens verskil betekenisvol. gem. fo van mans vertoon 'n kurviliniere verloop met ouderdom, naamlik 'n betekenisvolle verlaging in gem. fo vanaf 8-10 jaar tot 3040 jaar, waarna geleidelike styging in gem. fo met verdere veroudering plaasvind. individuele verskille kom egter voor. die gem. fo van vroue verlaag ook vanaf die ouderdomsgroep 8-10 jaar tot en met 30-40 jaar, maar met toenemende ouderdom vind egter een van drie veranderinge plaas naamlik min verandering, 'n effense verlaging of eers 'n verlaging en dan 'n verhoging. geen statisties betekenisvolle verskille kom vir ppff, ppaf en h/r-verhouding van mans en dames voor nie. een normbasis kan dus vir mans en dames per ouderdomsgroep opgestel word. ppff en ppaf verhoog met veroudering terwyl die h/r-verhouding verlaag. individuele verskille kom egter, veral by die geriatriese populasie, voor. die vokaal /a:/ lewer meer konstante resultate op as die vokaal li:l vir ppff, ppaf en h/r-verhouding en behoort dus as basis vir ouderdomsnorme gebruik te word. fonasie met verhoogde luidheid veroorsaak hoer gem. fo-waardes, laer ppffen ppaf-waardes en hoer h/r-verhoudingswaardes as fonasie met normale luidheid. 'n normbasis vir die onderskeie parameters is daargestel op grond van die statistiese berekening van 95%-vertrouensintervalle, wat gebruik is om 'n skatting te maak van die onbekende populasiegemiddeld. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 104 k o b i e cloete, e m i l y g r o e n e w a l d , a n i t a v a n d e r m e r w e , j e a n b a d e n h o r s t s l o t die h u i d i g e s t u d i e is 'n k w a n t i t a t i e w e analise v a n sekere s t e m p a r a m e t e r s d.m.v. e g g . r e s u l t a t e v a n die studie dra b y tot die u p s p r a a k n a v o r s i n g s l a b o r a t o r i u m se b e s k i k b a r e a n a l i s e m e t o d i e k deur die daarstelling van 'n n o r m a t i e w e b a s i s (sien b y l a e s 1 tot 4) vir g e b r u i k i n die kliniese en navorsingsituasie. a l h o e w e l subjektiewe e v a l u a s i e v a n d i e s t e m s t e e d s n o o d s a a k l i k is, m o e t die s t e m o o k o b j e k t i e f g e a n a l i s e e r w o r d s o d a t perseptuele, o r g a n i e s e e n a k o e s t i e s e d a t a m e k a a r a a n v u l e n s o d o e n d e 'n m e e r o m v a t t e n d e d i a g n o s t i e s e e n t e r a p e u tiese m e t o d i e k d a a r s t e l . v e r w y s i n g s aronson, a.e. (1985). clinical voice disorders: an interdisciplinary approach. (2nd ed.). new york: thieme inc. baken, r.j. (1987). clinical measurement of speech and voice. boston: college-hill press. biever, d.m. & bless, d.m. (1989). vibratory characteristics of the vocal folds in young adult and geriatric women. journal of voice. 3(2), 120-131. bowerman, b.l. & o'connell, r.t. (1990). linear statistical models. an applied approach (2nd ed.). ohio: miami university. brown, w.s., morris, r.j. & michel, j.f. (1989). vocal jitter in young adult and aged female voices. journal of voice, 3(2), 113-119. childers, d.g. & lee, c.k. (1991). vocal quality factors: analysis, synthesis, and perception. journal of the acoustical society of america, 90(5), 2394-2410. chodzko-zajko, w.j. & ringel, r.l. (1987). physiological aspects of aging. journal of voice, 1(1), 18-26. colton, r.h. & casper, j.k. (1990). understanding voice problems: a physiological perspective for diagnosis and treatment. baltimore: williams & wilkins. colton, r.h. & conture, e.g. (1990). problems and pitfalls of electroglottography. journal of voice, 4(1), 10-24. fitch, j.l. (1990). consistency of fundamental frequency and perturbation in repeated phonations of sustained vowels, reading and connected speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 55, 360-363. glaze, l.e., bless, d.m. & susser, r.d. (1990). acoustic analysis of vowel and loudness differences in children's voices. journal of voice, 4(1), 37-44. gould, w.j. (1988). the clinical voice laboratory: clinical application of voice research. journal of voice, 1(4), 305309. b y l a e 1 n o r m a t i e w e a a n d u i d i n g s vir g e m . fo v a n m a n s en v r o u e fonasie v a n /a:/ guy, r.f., edgley, c.e., arafat, i. & allen, d.e. (1987). social research methods: puzzles and solutions. boston: allyn & bacon, inc. hollien, h. (1987). "old voices": what do we really know about them? journal of voice, 1(1), 2-17. horii, y. (1980). vocal shimmer in sustained phonation. journal of speech and hearing research, 23, 202-209. horii, y. & ryan. w j (1981). fundamental frequency characteristics and perceived age of adult male speakers. folia phoniatrica, 33, 227-233. kay elemetrics corp. (1989). dsp sona-graph(tm), model 5500/5500-1 operating manual issue e. pine brook, nj: kay elemetrics corporation. lieberman, r. (1963). some acoustic measures of the fundamental periodicity of normal and pathologic larynges. journal of the acoustical society of america, 35(3), 344353. linville, s.e., korabic, e.w. & rosera, m. (1990). intraproduction variability in jitter measures from elderly speakers. journal of voice, 4(1), 45-51. mcfarlane, s.c. & watterson, t.l. (1991). clinical use of the laryngograph and the electroglottogram (egg) with voice disordered patients. seminars in speech and language, 12, 108-113. mcglone, r.e. & hollien, h. (1963). vocal pitch characteristics of aged women. journal of speech and hearing research, 6, 164-170. orlikoff, r.f. (1990a). heartbeat-related fundamental frequency and amplitude variation of healthy young and elderly male voices. journal of voice, 4(4), 322-328. orlikoff, r.f. (1990b). the relationship of age and cardiovascular health to certain acoustic characteristics of male voices. journal of speech and hearing research, 33, 450457. orlikoff, r.f. &kahane, j.c. (1991). influence of mean sound pressure level on jitter and shimmer measures, journal of voice, 5(2), 113-119. sataloff, r.t., spiegel, j.r., carroll, l.m., darby, k.s. hawkshaw, m^j. &rulnick, r.k. (1990). the clinical voice laboratory: practical design and clinical application. journal of voice, 4(3), 264-279. shipp, t. & hollien, h. (1969). perception of the aging male voice. journal of speech and hearing research, 12, 703709. smit, g.j. (1983). navorsingsmetodes in die gedragswetenskappe. pretoria: haum-opvoedkundige uitgewers. steyn, a.g.w., smit, c.f. & du toit, s.h.c. (1987). moderne statistiek vir diepraktyk. pretoria: j.l. van schaik (edms.) bpk. | wilcox, k.a. & horii, y. (1980). age and changes in >vocal jitter. journal of gerontology, 35(2), 194-198. j yumoto, e. (1988). quantitative assessment of the degree of hoarseness. journal of voice, 1(4), 310-313. ι v e r s k i l l e n d e o u d e r d o m s g r o e p e t y d e n s n o r m a l e en h a r d e vokaal /a:/ o u d e r d o m s g r o e p g e m . fo ( h z ) m e t n o r m a l e fonasie g e m . fo ( h z ) m e t h a r d e ' fonasie vokaal /a:/ o u d e r d o m s g r o e p l a a g s t e w a a r d e h o o g s t e w a a r d e l a a g s t e w a a r d e h o o g s t e w a a r d e m a n s 8 1 0 19-23 3 0 4 0 5 0 6 0 70-80 237.3 99.2 108.3 8 4 . 4 111.2 290.7 150.4 126.6 167.5 181.6 2 4 0 . 8 103.7 125.9 9 4 . 9 122.9 3 1 0 . 2 9 164.1 140.7 191.4 ' 198.5 8 1 0 2 5 0 . 1 3 1 4 . 2 2 7 4 . 2 , 3 3 8 . 1 19-23 216.8 3 2 3 . 5 2 2 1 . 3 / 3 3 0 . 1 v r o u e 3 0 4 0 194.9 2 4 9 . 7 196.1 2 4 9 . 4 5 0 6 0 175.7 2 7 1 . 5 2 0 2 . 9 2 8 1 . 3 7 0 8 0 174.9 2 2 1 . 2 184.2 / 2 4 1 . 0 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η elektroglottografiese analise van sekere stemparameters 105 bylae 2 normatiewe aanduidings vir die hoogste en laagste ppff-waardes ("jitter") vir persone in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe tydens normale en harde fonasie van /a:/ ppff ( %) met ppff (%) met harde normale fonasie fonasie vokaal ouderdomslaagste hoogste laagste hoogste groep waarde waarde waarde waarde 8-10 0.251 0.464 0.194 0.423 19-23 0.212 0.27 0.163 0.221 /a:/ 30-40 0.184 0.484 0.123 0.208 50-60 0.228 0.459 0.216 0.397 70-80 0.381 0.975 0.318 0.609 bylae 3 normatiewe aanduidings vir die hoogste en laagste ppaf-waardes ("shimmer") vir persone in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe tydens normale en harde fonasie van /a:/ ppaf (db) met ppaf (db) met harde normale fonasie fonasie vokaal ouderdomslaagste hoogste laagste hoogste groep waarde waarde waarde waarde 8-10 0.185 0.391 0.173 0.328 19-23 0.158 0.305 0.132 0.244 /a:/ 30-40 0.226 0.479 0.154 0.269 50-60 0.224 0.461 0.175 0.453 70-80 0.273 0.614 0.204 0.452 bylae 4 normatiewe aanduidings vir die hoogste en laagste h/r-verhoudingswaardes vir persone in verskillende ouderdomsgroepe tydens normale en harde fonasie van /a:/ h/r-verhouding (db) normale fonasie h/r-verhouding (db) vokaal ouderdomsgroep laagste waarde hoogste waarde laagste waarde hoogste waarde /a:/ 8 1 0 19-23 30-40 50-60 70-80 15.3 14.9 15.2 13.2 8.9 20.7 20.1 19.9 17.7 17.1 17.4 16.6 15.9 12.8 10.7 21.7 20.4 20.1 17.9 17.8 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, and critically evaluative theoretical and philosophical conceptual issues dealing with aspects of human communication and its disorders; service provision; training; and policy. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. manuscript style and requirements manuscripts should be accompanied by a covering letter providing the author's address and telephone numbers. all contributions are required to follow strictly, the style specified in the publication manual of the american psychological assoc. (3rd ed., 1983xapa pub. man.), with complete internal consistency. four copies of triple-spaced high quality type-written manuscripts with numbered pages, and wide margins should be submitted. they should be accompanied by one identical disc copy of the paper; (1) in wordperfect 5.1 (with an extension ,wp5). filenames should include the first author's initials and a clearly identifiable keyword or abbreviation thereof and should be typewritten on the last line of the last page of the reference list (for retrieval purposes only). as a rule, contributions should not exceed much more than 30 pages, although longer papers will be accepted if the additional length is warranted. the firs page of two copies should contain the title of the article, name of author(s), and institutional affiliation (or address). in accordance with the αρα pub. man. style (1833, p.23) authors are not required to provide qualifications. in the remaining two copies, the first page should contain only the title. the second page of all copies, should contain only an abstract (100 words), written in english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors. major headings where applicable should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements, references. all paragraphs should be indented. tables and figures which should be prepared on separate sheets (one per page), should be copied for review purposes and only the copies sent initially. figures, graphs, and line drawings that are used for publication however, must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper, but these will not be required until after the author has been notified of the acceptance of the article. lettering appearing on these should be uniform and professionally done, allowing for a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should appear below it and should not be included in the illustration. the titles of tables, which appear above, and figures, which appear below, should be concise but explanatory. both should be numbered in arabic numerals in order of appearance. the number of illustrative materials allowed, will be at the discretion of the editor (usually about 6). references references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and the date, e.g., van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, after the first occurrence, et al. after the first author will suffice, except for six or more when et al. may be used from the start. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list, which should be listed in strict alphabetical order in triple spacing at the end of the article. all references should be included in the list, including secondary sources, (αρα pub. man. 1983, p. 13). only acceptable abbreviations of journals may be used, (see dsh abstracts, october; or the world list of scientific periodicals). the number of references should not exceed much more than 30, unless specifically warranted. examples locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48 339-341. penrod, j.p. (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. davis, g.a. & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca: college-hill. editing acceptable manuscripts may be returned to the author for revision. additional minor changes may also be made at this stage, but a note on the manuscript acknowledging each alteration made by the author, is required. the paper is then returned to the editorial committee for final editing for style, clarity and consistency. reprints: 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. ι deadline for contributions: the preferred date is the 31st may each year, but papers will be accepted until 30th june by arrangement. | queries, correspondence & manuscripts: shiuld be addressed to the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech-language-hearing association, p.o. box 31782, braamfontein, 2017, south africa. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) inlicting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer verslae en artikels wat gemoeid is met navorsing, of handel oor krities evaluerende, teoretiese en filosofiese konseptuele kwessies wat oor menslike kommunikasie en kommunikasieafwykings; diensverskaffing; opleiding en beleid gaan. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings sal nie artikels aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. manuskrip styl en vereistes: manuskripte behoort deur 'n dekkingsbrief vergesel te word wat die skrywer se adres en telefoonnommers bevat. daar word van alle bydraers verwag om die styl, soos gespesifiseer is in die "publication manual of the american psychological assoc. (3rd ed., 1983) (αρα pub. man."), nougeset te volg met volledige interne ooreenstemming. manuskripte moet getik, van hoe gehalte en in drievoud spasiering met wye kantlyne wees. vier kopiee van die manuskrip moet verskaf word. een hiervan moet 'n identiese skyfkopie van die artikel wees in "wordperfect" 5.1 (met 'n uitbreiding ,wp5). leername behoort die eerste skrywer se voorletters en 'n duidelike identifiseerbare sleutelwoord of afkorting daarvan in te sluit en moet op die laaste lyn van die bladsy van die verwsyingslys getik word (slegs vir naslaan doeleindes). as 'n reel moet bydraes nie 30 bladsye oorskry nie, maar langer artikels sal aanvaar word indien die addisionele lengte dit regverdig. op die eerste bladsy van twee van die afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, naam van die skrywer(s), en instansie (of adres) verskyn. in ooreenstemming met die "αρα pub. man." se styl word daar nie van skrywers verwag om enige kwalifikasies te verskaf nie. op die eerste bladsy van die twee oorblywende afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskaf word. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. hoofopskrifte moet, waar van toepassing, in die volgende volgorde verskaf word: metode, resultate, besprekings, gevoljgtrekkings, erkennings en verwysings. alle paragrawe moet ingekeep word. tabelle en figure wat op afsonderlike bladsye (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie) moet verskyn, moet vir referent doeleindes gekopieer word en slegs die kopiee moet inisieel verskaf word. figure, grafieke en lyntekeriinge wat vir publikasie gebruik word, moet egter oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte wees. die oorspronklikes sal slegs verlang word nadat die artikel vir publikasies aanvaar is. letterwerk wat op bogenoemde verskyn, moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50% verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by illustrasies moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verklarings of legendes moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maar daaronder, verskyn. die opskrifte van tabelle (wat bo-aan verskyn), en die onderskrifte van figure, (wat onderaan verskyn), moet beknop, maar verklarend wees. numering moet deur middel van arabiese syfers geskied. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word. die aantal tabelle en illustrasies wat ingesluit word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). verwysings verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, b.v., van riper (1971). wanneer daar egter meer as twee skrywers is, moet daar na die eerste verskaffing van al die outeurs, van et al. gebruik gemaak word. in die geval waar daar egter ses of meer outeurs ter sprake moet et al 'van die begin af gebruik word. al die name van die skrywers moet in. die verwysingslys verskyn wat aan die einde van die artikel voorkom. verwysings moet alfabeties in trippel spasiering gerangskik word. al die verwysings moet in die verwysingslys verskyn, insluitende sekondere bronne, ("αρα pub. man." 1983, p. 13). slegs aanvaarbare afkortings van tydskrifte se titels mag gebruik word, (sien "dsh abstracts, october"; of the world list of scientific periodicals"). die aantal verwysings moet nie meer as 30 oorskrei nie, tensy dit geregverdig is. let op die volgende voorbeelde: locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341. penrod, j.p. (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. davis, g.a. & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca.: college-hill. rebigering manuskripte wat aanvaar is, mag na die skrywer teruggestuur word vir hersiening. addisionele kleiner veranderinge mag ook op hierdie stadium aangebring word, maar 'n nota ter aanduiding van alle veranderinge wat op die manuskrip voorkom, moet verskaf word. die artikel word dan aan die redaksionele komitee vir finale redigering van styl, duidelikheid en konsekwentheid teruggestuur. herbrukke: 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis aan die outeurs verskaf word. sluitingsdatum vir bydraes: bydraes word verkieslik teen 31 mei elke jaar verwag, maar artikels sal nog tot 30 junie vir aanvaarding oorweeg word. navrae, korrespondensie en manuskripte: moet geadresseer word aan die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, die suid-afrikaanse spraak-taal-gehoor vereniging, posbus 31782, braamfontein 2017, suid-afrika. diesuid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 77 early communication functioning of infants with cleft lip and palate alta kritzinger, brenda louw and rene hugo centre for early intervention in communication pathology department of communication pathology university of pretoria abstract this study investigated the early communication functioning and hearing abilities of 44 infants with cleft lip and palate, ages 3 to 31 months old. the results revealed that 64% of the subjects had a history of recurrent otitis media with effusion and 33% displayed associated anomalies. 26% of the subjects had mild hearing losses and middle ear pathology at the time of data collection. the subjects as a group displayed average developmental levels for perceptual-cognitive, socio-personal and receptive language skills, but a limited phonetic repertoire and a statistically significant expressive language delay. the results indicated that the subjects experienced a motor developmental delay, but this was not statistically significant. the implications for early communication intervention are to conduct regular hearing measurements and to conduct regular parent-centered therapy with individualized home programmes. efforts should be directed towards expanding early communication intervention services to include all infants with cleft lip and palate in south africa. opsomming: hierdie studie het ondersoek ingestel na die vroe'e kommunikasie funksionering en gehoorvermoens van 44 babas met gesplete lip en verhemelte, tussen die ouderdomme van 3 tot 31 maande. volgens die resultate het 64% van die proefpersone 'n geskiedenis van herhaalde otitis media met effusie en 33% het geassosieerde afwykings vertoon. 26% van die proefpersone het 'n geringe gehoorverlies en middeloor-patologie vertoon ten tye van data-insameling. die proefpersone as groep het gemiddelde vlakke van ontwikkeling vertoon in die areas van perseptuele kognitiewe ontwikkeling, sosiaal persoonlike ontwikkeling en reseptiewe taalvaardighede. hulle het egter 'n beperkte fonetiese repertoire en 'n staties beduidende ekspressiewe taalagterstand vertoon. alhoewel daar bevind is dat die proefpersone 'n motoriese ontwikkelingsagterstand vertoon het, is dit egter nie staties beduidend van aard nie. die implikasies vir vroe'e kommunikasie intervensie is om sulke babas se gehoor gereeld te monitor en om gereelde ouer gesentreerde terapie met geindividualiseerde tuisprogramme uit te voer. pogings moet aangewend word om vroee kommunikasie intervensie dienste uit te brei om alle gesplete lip en verhemelte babas in suid-afrika in te sluit. i key words: cleft lip andlpalate infants, risk factors, early communication development, early intervention. j infants with cleft lip and lpalate are at-risk of developing communication delays or disorders not only due to the established risk of the cleft itself, but also due to biological and environmental risk factors during their critical period for language acquisition. known biological risk factors which can negatively impact on the communication development of this population include associated anomalies and hearing impairment. although hearing impairment relative to recurrent otitis media with effusion has been reported extensively (goldman, martinez & ganzel, 1993; scheurle, 1989), the risk for sensorineural hearing impairment appears to be under reported. it is generally accepted that the presence of anatomical malformations of the head and neck, which includes the population with clefts, indicates a risk for all forms of hearing impairment (asha, 1994). one reason for the under reporting of sensorineural hearing loss could be that stud!es on the communication «development of children with cleft lip and palate often exclude the cases with associated anomalies who could present with various forms of hearing impairment. environmental risk factors proposed to impact on the early development of infants with cleft lip and palate include early surgical intervention, disruption of parent-infant-interaction due to the trauma of the diagnosis, resulting in changes in early socialization, the effect of infant appearance on adult developmental expectations and family stress due to increased care giving needs of having an infant with a congenital malformation (savage, neiman & reuter, 1994). the presence of multiple risk factors in infants with clefting conditions is indicative of a heterogenous population with the possibility of displaying a variety of developmental disorders. when applying the transactional view of etiology as described by samerhoff (1986), another implication becomes clear. the effect of multiple risk factors die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 alta kritzinger, brenda louw and rene hugo early in life implies a complex reciprocal interaction over time between infants with cleft lip and palate and their environment, resulting in an ever-changing profile of their speech and language development. for example, the transactional interaction of biological risk factors within the infant with a cleft lip and palate can be seen as the physiological malformation of the speech and hearing mechanisms (lynch, 1986) impacting on the neuromotor encoding and auditory decoding skills of the infant (bzoch in russell & grunwell, 1993), which may contribute to the speech and language delay evidenced in these infants. intermittent attacks of otitis media as well as discrete environmental events such as surgery resulting in family stress, add to a developmental profile which changes not only as a result of development itself. the transactional view of etiology also explains why no single risk factor can be viewed as the cause of early delays in speech and lantable 1. description of subjects, n=44 16% 21% 16% 40% 5 b i l a t e r a l c l p 6 u n i l a t e r a l c l p β s o f t p a l a t e id p i e r r e r o b i n • s u b m u c o u s • c l e f t l i p & s p figure 1: differences in cleft types, n=44 feature description age gender birthweight neonatal intensive care feeding during neonatal period associated conditions·. associated anomalies other physical defects low birthweight other biological risks total: range: 3 to 31 months mean: 15 months 24 subjects female (54,5%) 20 subjects male (45,5%) pierre robin sequence, n=7 range: 2,25kg to 4,25 mean: 2,94kg all other subjects, n=37 range: 1,37kg to 5kg mean: 3,23kg 10 subjects in incubator (22,7%) 34 subjects without intensive care (77,3%) 11 subjects tube fed (25%) 7 subjects breastfed (15,9%) 26 subjects bottle fed (59,1%) 1 subject with down syndrome 1 subject with sensorineural hearing loss due to congenital syphilis 1 subject with heart defect 1 subject with bilateral polydactyly 1 subject with hernia 8 subjects <2.5kg (18,2%) 1 subject one of a twin 14 subjects with associated conditions (31.9%) 30 subjects without associated conditions (68,1%) age of cleft repair soft palate: mean: 3 to 23 months 5 months lip and hard palate: mean: 6 to 9 months 7 months pre-operative early communication intervention (eci) socio-economic status 3 low income families (6,8%) geographical representation 41 subjects received eci (93,2%) 3 subjects received no eci (6,8%) 41 middle income families (93,2%) 28 living in gauteng province (63,6%) 16 living outside gauteng province (36,4%) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication functioning of infants with cleft lip and palate 79 -mage development in infants with cleft lip and palate and as a r e s u l t predictions of their developmental outcome cannot be accurate. in the light of the wide spectrum of risk factors intera c t i n g at different times during the development of infants with cleft lip and palate, early intervention is implem e n t e d as secondary prevention of speech and language delays. as preschool language development has shown to be the single best predictor of a child's school success (capute, palmer & shapiro, 1987), the aim of early interv e n t i o n is for comparable communication skills with peers a s young as possible. in spite of the known risk factors, l i m i t e d research has been conducted to provide a description of early communication profiles of infants with cleft lip and palate (savage et al., 1994). the ai*i of the study is to describe the early communication skills of a group of infants who attended an early intervention diagnostic clinic after being referred by the local cleft palate clinic attached to the university of pretoria or by professionals in private practice. methodology the research methodology took the form of a descriptive survey as discrete data was derived from observational situations during the chrib assessments. the subjects were recruited from the clinic for high risk babies (chrib) at the centre for early intervention in communication pathology, department of communication pathology, university of pretoria. chrib is a diagnostic facility of the early intervention programme for infants with disabilities or at-risk and their families and forms part of the centre's training activities and community service. anon-random non-probability sampling procedure was used to select the 44 subjects. description of subjects due to the descriptive nature of the research methodology, the characteristics of the subjects are discussed in detail as this information is viewed to be of importance in the interpretation of the results and the conclusions reached (savage, neiman & reuter, 1994). 44 infants with cleft lip and palate and cleft palate only served as subjects for the survey, displaying a variety of six different cleft types (see |figure 1). subjects with a cleft of the lip only were omitted from the study as they are generally not associated! with speech and language disorders (peterson-felzone, ;1989). an exceptional cleft type, namely a cleft of the lip and'soft palate with an intact hard palate, occurred in one subject. this subject presented with extremely low birth weight (1,37kg) and moderate prematurity (33 weeks gestation) as well. the age of the subjects depended on their referral to chrib, which were usually after their primary surgery was completed (see table 1). subjects with associated anomalies, such as the subject with down syndrome, were referred much earlier. the majority of the subjects (93%) had already received early communication intervention m the form of guidance and home programmes to the parents at the cleft palate clinic they were attending prior to their referral to chrib. the mean age of the subjects w a s 15 months which is considered as an ideal stage to conduct a comprehensive speech, language and hearing assessment and to continue with early communication intervention, as active team management usually decreases after primary surgery has been completed and clients may be lost for follow-up. according to table 1 the mean age of the subjects for soft palate repair was 5 months, while the mean age for lip and hard palate repair was 7 months. as indicated in table 1, the gender ratio displayed by the subjects does not reflect the accepted gender ratio of cleft lip and palate of twice as many males as females being affected. the omission of infants with a cleft lip only and the higher incidence of pierre robin sequence among females could be related to the gender differences found in the present study, i.e. slightly more females (54,5%) than males (45,5%) with cleft lip and palate. five of the seven subjects presenting with pierre robin sequence were female, which is in agreement with literature findings of a female bias among these children (amaratunga, 1989). the subjects' birth weight was recorded separately for the pierre robin sequence group and the rest of the subjects (see table 1). although the pierre robin sequence sample was too small to statistically compare them with the rest of the subjects, their mean birth weight of 2,94kg without prematurity should be noted. the subjects with pierre robin sequence displayed a lower mean birth weight than the rest of the subjects, as well as a lower birth weight compared to the general population in developed countries. (mean birth weight in developed countries: 3,4kg, ranging from 3,2kg to 3,8kg, according to turner, douglas & cockburn, 1988). the finding of a lower birth weight than the average among pierre robin se-. quence subjects can be compared to a study carried out by laitinen, heliovaara, pere and ranta (1994). this study also investigated the birth weight of pierre robin subjects and did not find a significant lower birth weight among them. the findings of the present study call for further research, as a better understanding of the characteristics of the pierre robin sequence population is of great clinical importance. apart from the subjects' birth weight, the need for incubator care also emphasizes the subjects' vulnerability for biological risk conditions. 22,7% of the subjects were kept in an incubator during the neonatal period. the listing of associated conditions in table 1 serves to provide a picture of the wide spectrum of major to minor associated anomalies and conditions present in 14 of the subjects. the characteristics of the subjects' feeding difficulties directly after birth, as shown in table 1, indicate that only 11 subjects required tube feeding. this implies that the subjects were referred for special care early enough to benefit from a jaw orthognathial suction and drinking plate (biitow, 1995), which resulted in successful bottle and even breast feeding. seven subjects succeeded in breast feeding, which is usually regarded as the least viable feeding option for infants with cleft palate. the last two items in table 1 concern the subjects' geographical and socio-economic backgrounds. 63,6% of the subjects lived in the gauteng province, a metropolitan area of which pretoria is one of the largest cities. most of the subjects were from middle income families, indicating that the study did not draw subjects from lower income families living in rural areas. this emphasises the inadequacies of tertiary health care to provide specialized services to infants with cleft lip and palate of families with limited resources to travel long distances to a cleft palate centre. at present the transformation of the national health system is to prioritize the development of primary health care suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 and community based rehabilitation services in the rural areas (anc, 1994). finally, the subjects' history of otitis media with effusion as a recurrent risk factor concerning their postnatal and later speech and language development, is presented in figure 2. 64% of the subjects had a history of recurrent otitis media with effusion. according to figure 3 they were treated either by the insertion of ventilation tubes or by medication. only 11% of the subjects received no treatment for otitis media, which implies that most of the parents were knowledgable about the management of middle ear infections. in summary, the subjects were between 3 and 31 months old and displayed a variety of cleft types which were already repaired at the time of data collection. their mean birth weight was within normal limits, but almost one third of the subjects either had associated anomalies and/or experienced biological risk conditions. recurrent otitis media with effusion occurred in almost two thirds of the subjects. most of the subjects were from middle income families living in a metropolitan area. assessment protocol and data collection each subject underwent a comprehensive assessment of their hearing abilities, prespeech and language skills and general development based on a communication assessment protocol for cleft palate infants developed by louw (1986) and adapted for chrib. the hearing assessment was carried out using sound field behavioural observation audiometry for subjects unalta kritzinger, brenda louw and rene hugo der 5 months developmental age and sound field visual response audiometry for subjects older than 5 months developmental age with narrow band noise as the test stimulus. immittance measurements were carried out for subjects of all ages. data on all hearing measurements were collected by a qualified audiologist with experience in pedoaudiometric procedures. normative data for different ages supplied by northern and downs (1984) was used to interpret the narrow band noise thresholds obtained from the subjects. subjects with abnormal middle ear measurements were referred to an ear, nose and throat specialist for follow-up. the subjects' prespeech and language skills and general developmental skills were elicited and observed in a controlled play context, involving the infant and a speechlanguage therapist with the infant's parents as onlookers. two qualified speech-language therapists with early intervention experience were involved in the data collection, one to elicit behaviour from the infant and the other to observe and record the data. infant behaviours such as vocalizations and verbalizations were recorded and used as descriptive data while other behaviours were recorded using a developmental scale, the developmental assessment schema (das) (anderson, nelson & fowler, 1978). data was analysed using descriptive statistics presented in the form of tables, pie charts and bar charts to provide means and ranges. the subjects' functioning on the das was statistically analysed using the friedman two-way analysis of variance test with bmdp 3s statistical software to determine significant differences between the 7 different subscales (level of significance: p_0.05). 32% 9% 23% 00 no otitis media 01-2x otitis media ® > 3 x otitis media 36% figure 2: history of otitis media, n=44 figure 3: treatment of otitis media, n=28 table 2. results of audiological assessment, n=44 normal thresholds elevated thresholds sensorineural hearing loss total hearing abilities 31 (72%) 11 (26%) 1 (2%) 44 normal middle ear pressure negative middle ear pressure / no test total middle ear functioning 15 (35%) 11 (26%) 17(40%) 44 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication functioning of infants with cleft lip and palate 81 results and discussion the developmental profiles of the subjects are presented a c c o r d i n g to the assessment areas of hearing abilities, prespeech skills and the different subscales of the das (anderson, et al., 1978), i.e. expressive language, receptive language, personal-social, perceptual-cognitive, gross motor, fine motor, and self help skills. hearing abilities table 2 indicates that 72% of the subjects displayed normal narrow band noise thresholds while 26% of the subjects showed elevated thresholds relative to mild hearing losses. one subject had a serious sensorineural hearing loss associated with prenatal syphilis exposure as well as a history of recurrent otitis media. at the time of testing she had ventilation tubes and no immittance measurements were carried out. the narrow band noise thresholds of all subjects could be successfully determined by the proposed testing procedures. not all subjects, however, could be tested for middle ear functioning. no immittance measurements were obtained of 17 subjects, as 15 subjects had ventilation tubes inserted following recurrent attacks of otitis media and two subjects would not cooperate for the immittance testing procedures to be carried out reliably. although 64% of the subjects (see figure 2) had a history of recurrent otitis media with effusion, only 26% of the subjects evidenced negative middle ear pressure at the time of data collection. the relative small number of subjects found with abnormal middle ear functioning could be due to the treatment received prior to the testing (see figure 3). the results also reveal the intermittent nature of the disease, indicating that reliable data on the occurrence of middle ear pathology among the subjects could only be obtained when data on both the history of the condition as well as immittance measurements were collected. this approach is also advocated by grunwell, sell and harding (1993). it is therefore concluded that a far greater number of subjects experienced recurrent middle ear pathology than the 26% of subjects gleaned from the immittance measurements, j as indicated in table 2, 11 subjects evidenced both elevated narrow band thresholds as well as negative middle ear measurements. although not statistically analysed to establish a correlation, the results suggest that those subjects with abnormal middle ear measurements may have experienced some form of mild hearing loss. it is generally accepted that a conductive hearing loss of 15 to 40db may accompany the different forms of otitis media with effusion, such as serious otitis media, acute otitis media and chronic otitis media (zarnoch & northern, 1989). the results emphasize the recurrent periods of auditory deprivation the subjects experienced during the critical window of opportunity for early language acquisition. these findings are in agreement with the studies of blakely and brockman (1995) and russell and grunwell (1993) who also found varying hearing levels associated with recurrent attacks of otitis media with effusion throughout infancy and early childhood as characteristic of their subjects with cleft lip and palate. prespeech and speech development the majority of the subjects (80%) displayed a limited phonetic repertoire in comparison with normal developmental levels as indicated in table 3. observations revealed that the limited phonetic repertoires of the subjects were characterized by vowels and nasal fricatives, with a distinct absence of plosives and to a lesser degree, fricatives. these findings are in agreement with chapman (1991) who found the number of consonants produced by 12 to 14 year old infants with cleft palate to be between 1 and 8, whereas non-cleft subjects displayed phonetic repertoires of 2 to 16 consonants. the most frequently occurring consonants of these subjects were /m/, /n/, /h/ and /w/. it is interesting to note that despite the similar findings of limited phonetic repertoires, the subjects of the chapman study (1991) had unrepaired cleft palates, whereas the present study utilized subjects with repaired cleft lip and palates. although the mean age of the subjects for soft palate repair in the present study was relatively early, i.e. 5 months (see table 1) most of the subjects displayed a limited phonetic repertoire. o'gara and logemann (1988) also found limited phonetic repertoires in their subjects with early palatal repair in comparison with normal development, suggesting that speech developmental constraints early in life continue to have an effect even after palatal repair. due to the limited expressive language displayed by the subjects, some as young as three months old, it was not possible to identify the resonance features of the prespeech utterances of 55% of the subjects (see table 3). it was, however, possible to detect hypernasality, identified as nasal resonance on vowels and substitutions of oral plosives and fricatives for nasals, in the prespeech and speech patterns of 25% of the subjects. 20% of the subjects evidenced both normal phonetic repertoires as well as normal resonance features in their prespeech and speech development. russell and grunwell (1993) also found a percentage (12%) of their subjects with cleft lip and palate to demonstrate normal phonetic development. results of the subscales of the das (anderson et al. 1978) figure 4 provides the results of the 7 subscales of the das (anderson, et al. 1978). the results of the subject's receptive language development indicate that a total of table 3. prespeech development, n=44 limited normal total phonetic repertoire 35 (80%) 9 (20%) 44 hypernasality normal could not establish total speech resonance | 11 (25%) 9 (20%) 24 (55%) 44 die suid -afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 alt a kritzinger, brenda louw and rene hugo 64% of the subjects demonstrated average skills. in comparison, the subjects' expressive language development indicates a marked delay, with only 32% subjects displaying normal development. table 4 provides the statistical analysis of the results, using the friedman two way analysis of variance test to perform multiple comparisons. this test demonstrated a significant difference between the results on the das expressive language scale (anderson, et al. 1978) and the other subscales, indicating a significant delay in the subjects' expressive language skills. the nature of the subjects' expressive language delay as assessed by the das (anderson, et al. 1978) includes delayed onset of vocalizations, babbling patterns, verbalizations, multiple word combinations as well as limited imitation skills. the subjects' delay in expressive language skills therefore includes more components of language abilities than the limited phonetic repertoires as discussed in the previous section. the present study therefore determined that the subjects displayed a delay in all components of expressive language development, indicating the extensive effects of the different risk factors on the subjects' early language development. the expressive language delay without a concurrent delay in receptive language development found in the subjects is in agreement with studies by louw (1986) and sherer and d'antonio (1995). louw (1986) found a communication developmental delay in subjects under 12 months of age and sherer and d'antonio (1995) found delays in expressive language development in their subjects with cleft lip and palate when compared with subjects without clefts. the subjects' age appropriate performance on personalsocial skills as measured on the das (anderson, et al. 1978) signifies no delay in their nonverbal communication interaction skills and personal attachment behaviours. a study by long and dalston (1982) extensively investigated the gestural communication skills of 12 month old infants with cleft lip and palate and compared them with a normal control group of subjects. the findings indicated that no significant differences existed between the two key: ε l: expressive language skills r l: receptive language skills p-s: personal-social skills p-c: perceptual-cognitive skills g m: gross motor skills f m: fine motor skills s h: self help skills • below β average mabove groups, demonstrating that infants with cleft lip and palate experience no delay in the development of their nonverbal expressive language skills. as receptive language ability and cognitive development are closely related in infancy (lahey, 1988), the subjects' average functioning on the perceptual-cognitive subscale of the das (anderson, et al. 1978) (see figure 2), is viewed as part of their language developmental profile. the subjects' language functioning in the present study was thus characterized by an incomplete phonetic repertoire, delayed expressive language development, but with age appropriate nonverbal communication interaction table 4. multiple comparisons performed for friedman two way analysis of variance test. critical ζ values indicating a significant difference: <3.04 for overall alpha of 0.5 (**). critical ζ values indicating a significant difference: <2.82 for overall alpha of 0.10 (*) figure 4: profile of development, n=44 comparisons ζ stat difference personal-social perceptual-cognitive 0.05 -1.00 personal-social , self help 0.89 18.00 personal social gross motor 1.53 31.00 personal-social fine motor 1.16 23.50 personal-social receptive language 2.42 49.00 personal-social expressive language 5.28** 107.00 perceptual-cognitive self help 0.94 19.00 perceptual-cognitive gross motor 1.58 32.00 perceptual-cognitive fine motor 1.21 24.50 perceptual-cognitive receptive language 2.47 50.00 perceptual-cognitive 1 expressive language 5.33** 108.00 self help gross motor 0.64 13.00 self help 1 5.50 fine motor 0.27 1 5.50 self help 1 receptive language 1.53 31.00 self help 1 expressive language 4.39** 39.00 gross motor fine motor 0.37 -7.50 gross motor receptive language 0.89 18.00 gross motor expressive language 3.75** 76.00 fine motor / receptive language 1.26 25.50 fine motor expressive language 4.12** 83.50 receptive language expressive language 2.86* 58.00 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early communication functioning of infants with cleft lip and palate 83 skills as well as average receptive language functioning and perceptual-cognitive skills. according to figure 2, 41% subjects displayed average gross motor, development, while most of the subjects (55 %) demonstrated average fine motor skills. although most subjects' gross motor skills were below average development, the statistical analysis did not reveal a significant difference between the various subscales to indicate a delay. not withstanding that research on the motor development of infants with cleft lip and palate is limited, savage et al. (1994) also suggest delayed motor development in 6 to 12 month old subjects as measured on the bayley scales of infant development. the results of the present study partly correlate with the findings of louw (1986) who determined delayed motor development in 44% of subjects, 0 to 12 months of age. the present study, utilizing older subjects, found fewer subjects (36%) functioning below average for motor development. longitudinal data is, however, needed to determine the developmental motor patterns of infants with cleft lip and palate. the last subscale of the das (anderson, 1978) deals with self help skills, i.e. skills related to feeding, dressing and toiletting. as a group the subjects displayed age appropriate developmental levels, indicating that feeding problems experienced before surgery were largely overcome by the time of data collection. in summary, the subjects' functioning on the 7 different subscales of the das (anderson, et al. 1978), were as follows: the majority of the subjects displayed age appropriate functioning in receptive language, perceptual-cognitive, personal-social and self help skills. their fine and gross motor skills evidenced lower levels of functioning, although these were not found to be statistically significant. the only developmental area in which the subjects as a group displayed a significant delay, was their expressive language skills. conclusion ι a characteristic communication developmental profile of the subjects emerged from the results. certain components of their language abilities revealed strong points, i.e. nonverbal communication interaction and receptive language skills, while an expressive language delay and a limited phonetic repertoire demonstrated their weak points. the subjects' communication profile should further be viewed against the broader developmental perspective of their age appropriate'cognitive and self help skills, but a slight delay in motor development was present. as the subjects had already benefitted from a communication intervention programme and sufficient management of middle ear pathology since birth, the results cannot be generalised, especially not to infants with cleft lip and palate from disadvantaged communities in south africa. in order to provide effective early intervention services in south africa for all infants with cleft lip and palate, further research is required. the effects of the risk factors associated with disadvantaged environmental circumstances on the communication development of these infants need to be determined. previous studies have extensively investigated the phonetic and phonological development of infants with cleft lip and palate (chapman, 1991; chapman & hardin, 1992; lohmander-agerskov, soderpalm, friede, persson & lilja, 1994; jansonius-schultheiss, 1989; o'gara & logemann, 1988; o'gara, logemann & rademaker, 1994; russell & grunwell, 1993), but much less focus was placed on the nature of these infants' language development. although the present study describes the nature of the subjects' expressive language delays, further research is required to provide clarity on the interaction between risk factors and language development. the study points to the need for widely available appropriate early intervention programmes for infants with cleft lip and palate. despite the dearth of research, however, some early language intervention programmes and protocols for infants with cleft lip and palate advocate a holistic approach and include all components of language development (langlois & nowak, 1990; lynch, brookshire & fox, 1993; scheuerle in bzoch, 1989). although these programmes are widely applied, they have to be adapted to meet the needs of the unique south african context. the current study provided a characteristic communication profile of the subjects as a group, but the diversity of the individual subjects should be emphasized. the presence of pierre robin sequence as a vulnerable group for hearing loss (handzic, bagatin, subotic & cuk, 1995), associated anomalies and malformations (sprintzen, 1988) and mental retardation among the subjects (cauettelaberge, bayet & larocque, 1994) calls for individualized language intervention programmes with full parental involvement and committed transdisciplinary teamwork. the incidence of associated conditions such as down syndrome and severe sensorineural hearing loss (see table 1) emphasizes the need for specialized knowledge in more than one field to meet the needs of infants with multiple disabilities. the intermittent hearing loss caused by recurrent otitis media with effusion which a number of subjects experienced, requires periodic monitoring of hearing at least every six months (asha, 1994). as a first attempt to describe a section of the cleft lip and palate population, the study provided useful information. expanded research is, however, necessary to provide information on which to base context relevant services. efforts must be directed towards expanding early communication intervention services to reach all infants with cleft lip and palate and their families. infants with cleft lip and palate represent a diverse population with various risk factors continuously influencing their communication development. they are particularly at-risk for expressive language delays. this poses great challenges for individualized early communication intervention services to them and their families. based on the results of the present study, the preferred approach is the implementation of regular early intervention sessions, commencing before surgery, fully involving the parents, with a curriculum emphasis not only on monitoring hearing abilities and facilitating expressive language development, but also on all components of language and general development. references amaratunga, n.a. de s. (1989). a comparative clinical study of pierre robin syndrome and isolated cleft palate. british journal of oral and maxillofacial surgery, 27, 451-458. anc. (1994). a national health plan for south africa. johannesburg: anc. anderson, d. nelson, j. & fowler, s. (1978). developmental assessment schema. in northcott, w.h. (ed.), curriculum die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 4 alta kritzinger, brenda louw and ren hugo guide: hearing impaired children (0-3 years) and their parents. washington, dc: the alexander bell association for the deaf, inc. asha. (1994). joint committee on infant hearing. 1994 position statement. asha, december, 38-41. blakely, r.w. & brockman, j.h. (1995). normal speech and hearing by age 6 as a goal for children with cleft palate: a demonstration project. american journal of speech-language pathology, 4(1), 25-32. butow, k-w. (1995). treatment of facial clef deformities. an illustrated guide. st. louis: ishiyalcu euro america, inc. capute, a. j., palmer, f.b. & shapiro, b.k. (1987). using language to track development. patient care, november, 60-71. cauette-laberge, l., bayet, b. & laroque, y. (1994). the pierre robin sequence: review of 125 cases and evolution of treatment modalities. plastic and reconstructive surgery, 93(5), 934-942. chapman, k.l. (1991). vocalizations of toddlers with cleft lip and palate. cleft palate cranio-facial journal, 28(2), 172178. chapman, k.l. & hardin, m.a. (1992). phonetic and phonological skills of two-year-olds with cleft palate. cleft palate craniofacial journal, 29(5), 435-443. goldman, j.l., martinez, s.a. & ganzel, t.m. (1993). eustachian tube dysfunction and its sequelae in patients with cleft palate. southern medical journal, 86(11), 1236-1237. grunwell, p., sell, d. & harding, a. (1993). describing cleft palate speech. in grunwell, p. (ed.), analysing cleft palate speech. london: whurr publishers ltd. handzic, j., bagatin, m. subotic, r. & cuk, v. (1995). hearing levels in pierre robin syndrome. cleft palate-craniofacial journal, 32(1), 30-36. jansonius-schultheiss, k. (1989). phonological development in two year old cleft lip and palate toddlers. proceedings: 6th international congress on cleft palate and related craniofacial anomalies, 29. jerusalem. laitinen,s.,heliovaara,a.,pere,a. &ranta, r. (1994). growth in children with pierre robin sequence and isolated cleft palate. acta paediatrics, 83, 1161-1164. lahey, m. (1988). language disorders and language development. new york: macmillan publishing company. langlois, a. & nowak, b.r. (1990). the first year of a child with cleft palate: an approach to facilitate communication development. infants and young children, 2(4), 34-50. lohmander-agerskov, α., soderpalm, e., friede, h., persson, e. & lilja, j. (1994). pre-speech in children with cleft lip and palate or cleft palate only: phonetic analysis related to morphologic and functional factors. cleft palate-craniofacial journal, 31(4), 271-279. long, n.v. & dalston, r.m. (1982). gestural communication in twelve-month-old cleft lip and palate children. cleft palate journal, 19(1), 57-61. louw, b. (1986). swart babas met gesplete lip en verhemelte: 'n morfofunksionele studie. unpublished d phil dissertation: university of pretoria. lynch, j.i. (1986). language of cleft infants: lessening the risk of delay through programming. seminars in speech and language, 7(3), 255-268. lynch, j.i., brookshire, b.l. & fox, d.r. (1993). a curriculum for infants and toddlers with cleft palate. developing speech and language. austin: pro-ed. northern, j.l. & downs, m.p. (1984). hearing in children. baltimore: williams & wilkens. o'gara, m.m. & logemann, j.a. (1988). phonetic analysis of the speech development of babies with cleft palate. cleft palate journal, 25(2), 122-134. o'gara, m.m., logemann, j.a. & rademaker, a.w. (1994). phonetic features by babies with unilateral cleft lip and palate. cleft palate-craniofacial journal, 31(6), 446-451. peterson-felzone, s.j. (1989). speech disorders related to craniofacial structural defects. in northern, j.l. (ed.), study guide for the handbook of speech-language pathology and audiology. toronto: b.c. decker inc. russell, j & grunwell, p. (1993). speech development in children with cleft palate. in grunwell, p. (ed.), analysing cleft palate speech. london: whurr publishers ltd. samerhoff, a.j. (1986). environmental context of child development. the journal of pediatrics, 109(1), 192-200. savage, h.e., neiman, g.s. & reuter, j.m. (1994). a developmental perspective on assessment of infants with clefts and related disorders. infant-toddler intervention, 4(3), 221-234. scherer, n.j. & d'antonio, l.l. (1995). parent questionnaire for screening early language development in children with cleft palate. cleft palate-craniofacial journal, 32(1), 7-13. scheuerle, j. (1989). stimulating language development in infants and toddlers with cleft palate. in bzoch, k.r. (ed.), communicative disorders related to cleft lip and palate. boston: little brown and company. shprintzen, r.j. (1988). pierre robin, micrognathia and airway obstruction: the dependency of treatment on accurate diagnosis. international anesthesiology clinics, 26(1), 64-71. turner, t.l., douglas, j. & cockburn, f. (1988). grains care of the newly born infant. edinburgh: churchill livingstone. zarnoch, j.m. & northern, j.l. (1989). audiologic manifestations. in northern, j.l. (ed.), study guide for the handbook of speech-language pathology and audiology. toronto: b.c. decker inc. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 7 a tribute to professor myrtle aron lesley wolk, ph.d syracuse, university syracuse, new york professor myrtle aron is retiring from the department of speech pathology and audiology at the university of the witwatersrand after a long and illustrious career. she has attained outstanding recognition in the fields of speech pathology and audiology. her diverse interests and areas of expertise range from childhood and adult stuttering to the broad base of audiology. in addition to her status as a key figure in speech pathology and audiology in south africa, she has attained an international reputation as well. i write this tribute to professor aron from the perspective of student, colleague and friend. i have indeed been fortunate to know professor aron in all of these capacities, and i have valued her contributions to me, both professional and personal, on all of these levels. professor aron was an excellent teacher in the areas of audiology and research methodology. she is respected deeply for her knowledge and professional integrity. it was distinctly remarkable how she was able to keep abreast of current literature and trends in the field and impart these to her students, while at the same time organize the administrative workings of her department. the clinical training in her department ranks as clearly outstanding at an international level because she encouraged students to think independently and creatively, and to develop therapeutic plans without total reliance on a clinic supervisor. professor aron demanded high standards for both clinical and academic work. she encouraged students to be creative, flexible and, perhaps most importantly, to value their indep e n d e n c e and freedom to apply their knowledge and develop individual treatment regimens in order to best meet the needs of their patients. i also had the opportunity φ work with professor aron as a colleague at the university of the witwatersrand. my experiences as a lecturer there were all influenced to some extent by professor aron's overall guidance, approach to education, and devotion to our profession. she encouraged faculty members to work independently and take full responsibility for their lecture courses, yet was always open and available for advice and assistance. professor aron was a central figure in my professional training and#:areer development. finally, through many years of professional association, i have considered professor aron a friend, and i am grateful for the warmth and friendship built over time. she has an unusual ability to show personal interest and concern, and to provide invaluable guidance and support when needed. few teachers and administrators have exhibited the level of caring that professor aron has shown towards me. i deeply value her friendship at this level. professor aron is respected highly in south africa for her intellectual excellence, dedication to the profession, and the model of professionalism she provides. she has an insightful approach to problem-solving, and a deep perspective within the field. moreover, her wide-reaching dedication has also included a commitment to the training of both black and white south african students and to the treatment of children and adults with speech, language and/or hearing disorders, regardless of race or ethnic background. professor aron has been selfless in her contribution. retirement may be thought of as: a time of accomplishment, a time of satisfaction, a time of reflection, a time of sadness, a time of peace, a time of relaxation, a time of closure ... a time to move on to other things... on behalf of all the members of the south african speech and hearing association — those residing in and outside south africa and the members of the american speech and hearing association worldwide, i would like to wish professor aron a sense of fulfilment as she reflects on her contributions to the development of our profession in south africa. we wish her good luck, good health, and much happiness in her future. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasiafykins vol. 37. 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 professor m.l. aron 'n huldeblyk professor isabel uys departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria hoe fragmenteer 'n mens 'n kompleksiteit sonder om daaraan afbreuk te doen? hoe kan ek praat oor iemand soos professor aron sonder om chookie daarby in te sleep? 'n mulddimensionele, veelvlakkige mens is sy. 'n mens met wie altyd rekening gehou moet word dis sy. 'n stilte van haar kant af is 'n "op julle merke"-sein vir almal om haar. en dit is een van haar grootste bates, waarvoor sy waarskynlik nognooitgenoegerkenninggekry hetnie. sy laatnie dinge met haar gebeur nie, maar sorg dat dinge om haar gebeur tot voordeel van almal. dit is die boodskap van haar lewe vir ons as kollegas en vriende. as professionele leier het sy oorgeneem na die vestiging van die kursusse en die beroep. maar sy het gesorg vir erkenning, vir uitbouing en aanvaarding. was daar al ooit iemand wat met soveel toewyding dag vir dag kon stry vir spraakterapie en oudiologie in suid-afrika? sy deins nooit terug vir struikkelblokke en uitdagings nie en dikwels was dit al nodig om op 'n nie so subtiele manier die bul by die horings te pak. sy is braaf, ja. om hierdie eensame paadjie te loop is nie altyd maklik nie, maar dit is juis omdat sy as mens vrede gemaak het met haar eie kompleksiteit wat sy daartoe in staat was. een oomblik kan sy hoogs gesofistikeerd die koue wetenskaplike feite aanhaal, om net daarna met 'n nai'we humorsin te vertel hoe haar besonder intelligente hond die telefoon antwoord. sodra jou aandag goed afgetrek is, sou sy bewys lewer dat jou afleibaarheid nie 'n goeie professionele eienskap is nie. nie maklik nie, maar 'n waardevolle les. stres en "burn-out" het in ons beroep al mode geword. waarom het hierdie simptome nog nooit by haar kop uitgesteek nie? omdat haar beroep ook 'n roeping is, 'n lewenstaak wat sy met liefde, toewyding en entoesiasme uitvoer. dit is vir haar lekker om te werk en haar entoesiasme is aansteeklik. een van haar grootste bates in die beroep is haar vermoe om ander entoesiasties te maak en te inspireer nie net om iets te doen nie, maar om dit na die beste van jou vermoe te doen. dit is ook waar die vriendin inkom. professor aron kan jou aan die werk kry, maar chookie sal altyd lojaal bly, jou beskerm, help en ondersteun. sy gee krediet waar dit toekom en tel op as jy geval het. sy het die gawe van die spreekwoordelike ystervuis wat in watte toegerol is. nou het sy as departementshoof afgetree, maar soos ons al almal agtergekom het, is dit maar net een van die verantwoordelikhede wat sy verruil het vir 'n hele paar ander. nou het die tyd gekom vir die uitlewing van al haar ander belangstellings. gaan die oudioloog weer die spraakterapeut toelaat om na vore te tree? gaan die administrateur weer vir 'n rukkie plek maak vir die navorser? wat ook al gaan gebeur, ons gaan nog daarvan moet kennis neem. sy het nog soveel om by te dra en haar bydraes kan nie gei'gnoreer word nie. dit was my boodskap oor haar. maar ek het ook 'n boodskap vir haar. ons is reeds in die "op julle merke"-posisie. ek weet sy is "gereed". ons wag nou vir die "gaan"! the south afi icon journal of communication .disorders, vol. '37. 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 9 tribute to professor aron marcelle norman chairman, saslha professor aron has been involved with the south african speech-language-hearing association (saslha) since her days as a student speech therapist when our organisation was still called the s a logopedics society. she has held most portfolios in the organisation but her function as editor of the journal, chairman and in the past eight years as president, are the positions that we all best remember her in. her contribution to our profession has extended far beyond her role in the south african speech-languagehearing association, her position as professor of the department of speech pathology and audiology at the university of the witwatersrand and her chairmanship of the professional board for speech pathology and audiology with the south african medical and dental council (samdc). she has been involved in or responsible for every major change and advancement that has taken place within our profession. in addition, we have had to deal with frequent and very serious threats from many overseas speech and hearing associations who have attempted to have us barred from the international association for logopedics and phoniatrics (ialp). once again professor aron has represented us at these international meetings as well as at conferences in america and elsewhere where she has put our case and ensured our continued participation in and recognition by professional organisations around the world. it is said that no-one is ever indispensible but as far as saslha is concerned, professor aron is indeed indispensible. her knowledge is encyclopaedic and her memory frightening and she is the person we turn to for the confirmation of all major decisions and for the elusive details of history that professional groups often need to draw on. we know that professor aron is only retiring her position as head of department from the university of the witwatersrand and that she is certainly not giving up her involvement with saslha and the professional board but we felt that this was a fitting moment to pay her a long overdue tribute. her commitment to saslha and her profession of speech therapy and audiology on every level has been extraordinary. there can be few similar professions that have had one person who has played such a vital and profound role over such a long and unbroken time period. to have a permanent reminder of her role in saslha we have decided to dedicate this year's journal as a commemorative issue in her honour and we wish her much happiness in her retirement. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 professor aron: a personal message aura kagan the speech and stroke centre north york, ontario, canada although we have not had any contact over the past few years, prof. aron and the department have often been in my thoughts. the first thing that i would like to mention (and i am sure that you have heard this over and over again) is the high calibre of graduates from the wits speech-pathology department. toronto is a city of immigrants and thus one meets professionals from all over the world. the graduates of our department enjoy an excellent reputation which reflects well on the training we received. much of this is due to prof. aron's dedication and enthusiasm and i think that we all owe her a personal debt of gratitude. i think that you may be interested in some of the work that i have been doing and the unique agency in which i am involved. the speech & stroke centre north york was started approximately 10 years ago by pat arato. pat's husband suffered a stroke and after the rehabilitation phase was over, she realized that this relatively young man was not going to be able to return to work and that there were no long-term facilities available. although she had absolutely no professional training, she started encouraging individuals with chronic aphasia to join communication groups led by volunteers. in these early days, there was some help from one or two speech-pathologists, but volunteers had to rely largely on their own resources. pat soon realized that something wonderful was happening to the people attending the groups. they seemed to be gaining confidence in using the communication skills that they had and seemed to feel better about themselves generally. the centre grew from this small venture to the sophisticated operation that it is today. we have over 100 aphasic members with a staff of 4 speechlanguage pathologists (2.5 positions) and about 60 volunteers. we also have a full-time volunteer co-ordinator and clerical staff. the philosophy of the program is what is probably of most interest. what we feel makes this centre unique is the combination of the following factors: 1) focus on giving individuals with chronic aphasia an opportunity to communicate, i.e. concentrating on ways of facilitating the exchange of information so that the focus is on what is being communicated rather than on how it is communicated. in the process of doing this, we have developed expertise in training lay individuals in communication strategies that help get communication flowing within a group format. the idea is not to replace what speechlanguage pathologists do but rather to supplement this with what we feel is a much neglected aspect of our work with aphasic clients using our skills to help them to forget, albeit for a short while, that they are aphasic and to concentrate on exchanging information, feelings and opinions with other individuals in a natural non-institutional atmosphere. 2) functional approach. 3) integration of community and professional resources (volunteers receive professional training and on-going, daily supervision). we work in extremely overcrowded conditions and desperately need more space. because we provide long-term support, we do not discharge members and thus, in order to cope with the rapidly expanding waiting-list, we have recently opened our first satellite centre and hope to be opening more in the future. as is the situation everywhere, there is an acute shortage of funds and this is a major problem. activities in which i have been involved include: trying to formulate what we do so that we can share it with others. this is easier said than done as many of the variables we look at are in the psycho-social realm and are extremely difficult to measure. establishing credibility within the professional community at first, many professionals were wary about the concept of using volunteers, for obvious reasons. we now receive referrals from all the major hospitals in toronto and surrounding areas. in fact, our waiting list exceeds our present membership. we are also now an official training centre for the university of toronto and provide internships for students who are about to graduate. providing on-going professional education in the areas of aphasia. we have just had our 4th annual seminar and have also had numerous smaller events and open-houses inbetween. the feedback we have received has been extremely positive. some of the topics include: pragmatic and functional communication; the use of interactive drawing for establishing communication with expressively restricted aphasic adults; the challenge of generalization, and the art and science of aphasia therapy. speakers have included audrey holland, jon lyon, kevin reams, marianne simpson and sally byng. various research projects are underway, e.g. using new measures of functional communication to try and help document the type of change we see occurring in our aphasic members; developing our own measures to capture the psycho-social aspects relevant to the aphasic population and their families; investigating methods for formalizing our volunteer training so that other professionals can benefit from our experience as well as developing methodsto demonstrate that a volunteer has reached a certain level of proficiency. , i am fortunate to be working with a dedicated staff, two of whom are south africans. lorraine podolsky works parttime and rochelle cohen-schneider almost full-time. we all send greetings to you and invite you to visit us if you are ever in toronto. / r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 11 professor m.l. aron: continually in mind ... brenda jacks lewsen etobiooke, ontario canada prof aron to mask or not to mask, what to cook for supper, to prepare data for the statistician or read a book or, better still, browse through the latest gardening catalogue which got dumped on the doorstep. the list goes on ... prof you're wondering where this is all leading or what it could possibly have to do with you. please bear with me a little longer, because although you are entirely unaware of the fact, you are frequently involved with many of my daily decisions. over the years, i suppose, we all develop our own odd ways of dealing with decisions, or matters of principle, or prickly twinges of conscience. i always find myself in a witness box facing a jury. the members of my mental juries vary depending on the nature of the problems to be solved. here's an example when i've been particularly unreasonable with my kids i evoke the ire of the jury which includes my mother, of course, the occasional humanist and a well known canadian writer who feels strongly about children's rights. when it comes to professional or audiological issues the jury box is shared by my current director, sometimes a physician and naturally you, yourself. the temptations tp cut research corners, do sloppy audiometry, or to conduct corridor conferences about patients result in no-no responses of the conscience stirring variety. but rigour innovation, careful attention to detail whether at the audiometer or while supervising colleagues and my entire jury smiles benignly. the good news for me is that there is no retirement from doing jury duty in my head. the bad news is that it is a real pity that i am forever playing these mind games. it would be so much nicer to be able to discuss issues and kick ideas around with you, face-to-face, more often. this is a long and public way of saying that i'm thinking of you and looking forward to seeing you. 3 play m a n d • j schoolroom shop 6l the rosebank mews 173 oxford road rosebank jhb. play & schoolroom, specialists in the field of child education have been offering assistance to both professionals and parents for nearly thirty years. their expertise and advice range through preschool education, perceptual training, primary and remedial education and adult education. play and schoolroom are sole agents for learning development aids which include an excellent selection of materials of interest to the speech therapist. they also offer an interesting range of aids and books to foster and develop language and communicative skills. their stock of educational books and toys is exceptionally wide. you are invited to view their superb range in their new beautifully laid out showroom. phone 7 8 8 1 3 0 4 fax: 8 8 0 1 3 4 1 ι po box 5 2 1 3 7 s a x o n w o l d 2 1 3 2 die sidd-afrikaansc tydskrif \ir kom»wiiikttsictifirykiii/is. vol. 37. 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 professor aron : an appreciation hanna klein, b.a. (speech and hearing therapy) m.a. (clinical audiology) witwatersrand. mcst co-ordinator, department of continuing education national hospitals college of speech sciences london, united kingdom miss aron, as she then was, first appeared to me operating a strange machine at the international fair, held at wits, many, many years ago. overwhelming images of careers and artifacts, which were totally new, crowded in on my adolescent, impressionable mind. professor aron remained a particular image — small, determined, articulate: a professional woman espousing new concepts in a scientific mode. the image of this idealized woman remains to this day. an uncompromising figure of impeccable intellectual, political and academic integrity, professor aron has mothered endless streams of women into a profession that has continued to develop, change and enlarge its horizons. her great asset of intense loyalty and respect for all the peoples of southern africa has been the bedrock of my respect for her. guilt, at having deserted her framework of commitment to the provision of the most excellent services to all those in need of them, by living abroad, is all pervasive. is it any comfort to professor aron that her trainees are highly respected for the calibre of the wide range of contributions they have made to many aspects of communication disorders, in major centres all over the world? the department of speech pathology at wits must stand as a monument to her grit and determination that south african students would have access to facilities and training that matched any unit in the world. her ideas and practical skills flourished, and are now deployed with great enthusiasm, by many graduates, elsewhere, away from the nest of support and encouragement. but outside of the "department" and in addition to her standing within the university as a whole, professor aron is widely acknowledged and admired for her passionate, and long standing fight to keep sasha within the international arena of professional activity, despite some vicious orchestrations to misrepresent the position of south african speech therapists and audiologists, and their clients, within the south african context. i am proud to be a wits graduate. my professional development is due almost entirely to the vision, standpoint and influence of a true woman of worth. she made us believe through an outstanding training, that we could enhance the communication skills in all those deprived of them, through accurate observation, pertinent treatment,and the consideration, in the first instance, that all men, women and children were deserving of an equality of care and consideration. / 1'lic south african journal of'coiiniiiiuication disorders, vol. '.\7. 19u0 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 13 a personal tribute to prof. chookie aron marion fredman haifa, israel prof. chookie aron had more influence on my professional career than any other individual. it may even be that her influence on me was greater than on most of the students who have been under her supervision throughout the years. in 1950 when i started studying at wits, i could not decide which profession to choose. in my first year i took courses which would leave my options open for future study. i knew chookie previously and met her again on the campus. when i told her about my dilemma she suggested that i come to the speech clinic in the third basement of the library and discuss speech therapy with her. on my first visit i observed her treating a case and shortly after that my mind was made up. i qualified in 1954 and in 1958 emigrated to israel. in 1965 i was offered an american government research grant to carry out a project on adult aphasics. it was a wonderful opportunity to work towards an m.a. but as yet there was no speech therapy department in israel. so i turned to chookie and was accepted at wits for my degree under her supervision. during the four years that i carried out the project and wrote my thesis, i only visited south africa once or twice so that all my supervision was by correspondence. it is only due to chookie's endless patience and willingness to help that i graduated in 1970. her task not only involved supervising my work but included helping me borrow books from the wits library. this was no mean feat in the pre-computer-fax era! throughout the years chookie and i have been meeting and cooperating as delegates to the i.a.l.p. congresses where each of our countries need all the support they can get. our most recent encounter in prague was a truly historic one. we managed to see the city together three mon ths before the revolution which has changed the country considerably. i wish chookie a happy retirement. i know that she will continue to work for the benefit of the speech and hearing impaired community wherever she is needed. professor aron : a message from a distance marlene behrmann marlene carno jacobson danielle kaplan toronto, canada it is with feelings of the utmost respect, admiration and gratitude that we write this tribute from toronto to professor m.l. aron. | / for us, prof. aron was a mentor, teacher and a sensitive, caring person. she nurtured the development and advancement of speech-language pathology and audiology in the south african milieu over several decades, a singular accomplishment for one person. her vision of our field was constantly attuned to the changing needs of the country and she was instrumental in redirecting the focus of the discipline to accommodate such changes with remarkable flexibility. furthermore, she was able to foresee and anticipate the need for change, as manifest in her day-to-day activities and long-range plans. on the academic front, prof. aron insisted that students receive a broad, rich foundation, covering all facets of communication disorders, thereby enabling her graduates everywhere to succeed in diverse areas of specialty. while attending to the acute grass-roots needs of the profession and the country, prof, still saw the importance of a postgraduate program, which could ensure our participation internationally. her breadth extended still further: she implemented a database of tremendous scope, targeting the unique epidemiology of communication disorders in south africa; she also ventured beyond the academic ivory tower into the unknowns of rural health-care and at the same time, managed to maintain close and personal links with her many students and colleagues. as recipients of prof.'s generous spirit, continued interest and hospitality, we take this opportunity to pay tribute to a woman ahead of her time. prof, we salute you and wish you fulfilment of your many dreams and endeavours. die sitid-afiiktumsc ti/dskrif n'r kommunikasicafiqiuhuis, vol. 37. 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 reflections on prof. chookie aron margaret marks wahlhaus johannesburg the phrase "full circle" carries with it pleasing connotations of satisfying completeness. it is an apt description of professor aron's academic life and achievements. my nearly forty year's knowledge of chookie aron as a friend and colleague (we were both very young at the time) gives me a privileged perspective of the circles within circles which have gone up to make the interest and world of this remarkable person. i would write at length of the way that her interest in art and in music have been honed and rounded by her educated eye and ear and her commitment to do and find the best to enjoy and appreciate, whether in pottery, photography, piano performance, architecture, or the creation of beauty in her home and in her garden. but perhaps the full circle can best be highlighted by her involvement with the people of south africa. from her time as a student, chookie was concerned on an active and practical level with the people who had no voice, or muted voices a natural care for a speech therapist. her interest in the arts led her, through an involvement with the university of the witwatersrand's arts festival (which she chaired in a frantic but fulfilled final "logopedics" year) to work with union artists, a group which was established to give opportunities to black artists and musicians and was the inspiration of much of the artistic work enjoyed today. her master's dissertation, on stuttering in the black population, has become a source work in the field. all her interests in the arts, in the university where she had studied and taught, in the people of this country, and particularly in those who have communication disorders, culminated in her dedicated concern which led to the establishment of a course at the university of the witwatersrand, for the training of community workers in speech and hearing therapy. her enthusiasm and involvement have ensured attention for so many of the communicatively impaired people, particularly those in the rural and underprivileged areas of southern africa who would otherwise have no help. and so the circle of her life and interest is continuing, and her retirement is just a beginning of more commitment and involvement with the principles, standards and ethics of our profession, with the community workers, and with the urgent needs of the people of our land. as the work of a teacher can never be fully measured, so the influence of a person and teacher such as chookie aron can never be limited. the circles grow wider ... / ίίιι· south af'rir/iii foiini/if of coiiimiuiiaitioii disortlcrs, vol. 37. 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 31 die aard en voorkoms van middeloorpatologiee in laerskole vir normaalhorende blanke kinders carin cilliers, β log (pretoria)* deon ρ rossouw, mb ch β μ med (orl) (pretoria) santie meyer, μ log (pretoria)* marina hurter, β log (pretoria)* *departement spraakheelkunde, spraakwetenskap en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria departement oor-, neusen keelheelkunde, universiteit van pretoria opsomming die doel van hierdie studie is om vas te stel wat die aard en voorkoms van middeloorprobleme by graad 1 en 2 blanke kinders is. siftingsoudiometrie is op 50 leerlinge (100 ore), in 'n hoe, en 50 leerlinge (100 ore) in 'n lae sosio-ekonomiese omgewing uitgevoer. alle leerlinge wat 'n eerste oudiometriese siftingsprosedure gefaal het, is aan 'n tweede siftingsprosedure onderwerp. uit die resultate blyk dit dat 19,5% uitgeval het op die oudiologiese siftingstoetse. 'n otologiese ondersoek het 8,5% van hierdie kinders geidentifiseer as kinders met middeloorprobleme. daar was nie statisties beduidende verskille tussen die verskillende sosio-ekonomiese groepe nie. abstract the aim of the study was to determine the prevalence of middle ear dysfunction amongst grade 1 and 2 white children. identification audiometry was performed on 50 children (100 ears) in a high, and 50 children (100 ears) in a low socio-economic area. all children failing the identification audiometric procedure were re-evaluated. the results indicated that 19,5% also failed the second audiometric screening. of these children 8,5ψο were identified as children with middle ear problems. there were no statistically significant differences between the two socio-economic groups. voor die ouderdom van ti'en jaar het ongeveer 75% van alle /'kinders een of meer aanvjalle van akute otitis media ondervind (m611er, 1985). juis aangesien daar so 'n hoe voorkoms van otitis media is, is vroee identifisering, soos by enige gehoorpatologie, van groot belang. die rede hiervoor is dat dit gevolge kan he wat deur vroee intervensie voorkom kan word. met ander woorde, kinders wat herhaalde aanvalle van chroniese otitis media in die vroee lewensjare gehad het, kan later as gevolg hiervan sekondere probleme toon. dit sluit onder andere in: vertraagde taalontwikkeling en versteurde taalvaardighede, ouditiewe perseptuele probleme, swak intellektuele vermoens, swak akademiese prestasie en gedragsen emosionele probleme (hugo, 1984; feldman en wilber, 1977; northern en downs, 1984). die rede vir die negatiewe sekondere gevolge van otitis media is dat kinders 'n wisselende graad van ouditiewe deprivasie ondervind, as gevolg van die fluktuerende aard van die toestand. sekere hoerisikogroepe is al geidentifiseer, byvoorbeeld die gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltepopulasie (mcwilliams, morris en shelton, 1984). daar is byvoorbeeld ook uitgevind dat die voorkoms van otitis media opvallend hoer in die laer sosio-ekonomiese groepe as in die hoer sosioekonomiese groepe is (klein,yl978). dit kan waarskynlik aan verskeie faktore toegeskryf word, byvoorbeeld oorbevolking, swak sanitere toestande, ontoereikende mediese die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 dienste, wanvoeding, sosio-kulturele ongelykheid, lae motivering en swak algemene gesondheid (lewis, 1976; klein, 1978). alhoewel daar reeds baie navorsing gedoen is ten opsigte van kindergehoorverliese in eerste wereldlande, byvoorbeeld swede, engeland en die verenigde state van amerika, bestaan daar 'n groot leemte ten opsigte van inligting oor derde wereldlande (wilson, 1985). suid-afrika, met die kenmerkende eerste en derde wereldomstandighede, vertoon ook 'n gebrek aan inligting ten opsigte van die prevalensie van ooren gehoorpatologie. geen studies in hierdie verband is al op die suid-afrikaanse kaukasiese groepe (waaronder die blankes gereken word) gedoen nie. hierdie studie is dus 'n poging om ten dele hierdie leemte te vul en vas te stel wat die voorkoms van middeloorpatologie onder suid-afrikaanse graad 1 en 2 blanke kinders is en ook om vas te stel of sosio-ekonomiese verskille wel 'n merkbare invloed uitoefen op die voorkoms van middeloorpatologiee. eksperiment proefpersone blanke kinders woonagtig in pretoria is geselekteer. die © sasha 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 carin cilliers, deon rossouw, santie meyer en marina hurter kinders is in graad 1 en graad 2. een groep besoek 'n skool wat in 'n lae sosio-ekonomiese gebied gelee is. die ander groep besoek 'n skool in 'n hoe sosio-ekonomiese area (van bergen, 1982). die kinders is deur middel van 'n gestratifiseerde ewekansige steekproef uit die 2 skole geselekteer. tabel 1: voorstelling van proefpersone ouderdom skool 1* skool 2** totaal 6 jaar 16 9 25 7 jaar 34 35 69 8 jaar 0 6 6 totaal 50 50 * skool 1: hoer sosio-ekonomiese status ** skool 2: laer sosio-ekonomiese status apparaat die otoskopiese ondersoek is deur middel van 'n welch allyn otoskoop en immittansiemetings deur middel van 'n grason stadler model gsi 28 auto tymp (geyk volgens 150-1975 standaarde) uitgevoer. die suiwertoondrempels is met behulp van madsen electronics ob40 (geyk volgens sabs 082 standaarde) bepaal en die agtergrondgeraas is met behulp van 'n bruel en kjaer 2204 klankpeilmeter gemeet. prosedure die eksperimentele metode is 'n tweegroepontwerp wat 'n tussengroep ontwerpmetode is (smit, 1983). al die proefpersone is in april en mei getoets aangesien klimaatverandering die resultate kan bei'nvloed wanneer boonste lugweginfeksies in die wintermaande die voorkoms van die middeloorpatologie verhoog (voogt, halama & van der merwe, 1986). klankpeilmetings is van 'n stil vertrek gemaak om te bepaal of dit geskik vir suiwertoonsiftingsdoeleindes is (barrett, 1985). in beide skole was die agtergrondgeraas in die vertrek binne die voorgestelde perke en dus geskik vir siftingsoudiometrie. 'n inisiele gehoorsifting is uitgevoer om kinders sonder middeloordisfunksie uit te skakel. hiervoor is al die proefpersone aan 'n otoskopiese ondersoek en siftingsimmittaiisiemeting onderwerp. slegs proefpersone wat nie die inisiele siftingstoets geslaag het nie, is aan opvolgtoetsing onderwerp. die opvolgtoetsing het bestaan uit 'n oor-, neusen keelondersoek, uitgevoer deur 'n oor-, neusen keelarts, siftingsimmittansiemetings en 'n suiwertoonsiftingsondersoek. data-ontleding die ondersoek van die timpaniese membraan is na aanleiding van die oor-, neusen keelarts se bevindings as volg geklassifiseer: i — normaal, ii — geskend (fibroties, atrofies, skleroties), iii — kliniese otitis media (cholestiatoma, timpaniesemembraan-perforasie, middeloor-effusie, atelektatiese otitis media, akute otitis media). siftingsimmittansiemetings se slaag/faalkriteria is soos volg (asha in barrett, 1985): klas i — slaag middeloordruk tussen + 100 en 200 dapa en 'n teenwoordige akoestiese refleks. klas ii — risiko abnormale middeloordruk en teenwoordige akoestiese refleks of middeloordruk tussen + 100 en 200 dapa en afwesige akoestiese refleks. hertoets na drie tot vyf weke. klas iii — faal middeloordruk abnormaal en afwesige akoestiese refleks. die kriteria vir normale en afwykende timpanogramme is volgens jerger (1970) ter aanvulling van bogenoemde kriteria gebruik. suiwertoonsiftingskriteria is as volg (asha in barrett 1985): volgens die standaarde moet proefpersone drempels he van: 20 db gp by 500 hz, 1000 hz en 2000 hz en 20 db of 25 db by 4000 hz. om te bepaal of die verskille tussen die twee groepe proefpersone statistics beduidend is, is daar varrdie volgende formule gebruik gemaak: pi p2 s/p]i-pj~ s/ϊ/οο+τ/ϊ00 waardes is op die^to peil van betekenis bereken. resultate tabel 2: die ore wat die inisiele en opvolgimmitansiemetings slaag/faal faal inisiele faal opvolgsifting sifting skool 1: 29/100 20/100 | ν = 100 (29 %) (20 %) skool 2: 24/100 19/100 ; ν = 100 (24 %) (19 %) ] totaal: 53/200 39/200 1 ν = 200 (26,5 %) (19,5) ! in tabel 2 word 'n vergelyking getref tussen die aantal pre wat die eerste sifting gefaal het en die wat die tweede sifting gefaal het. uit tabel 2 blyk dit dat 19,5% ore die tweede sifting gefaal het waar daar aanvanklik 26,5% ore gefaal het. daar het dus meer leerlinge gefaal tydens die eerste toetsing as tydens die tweede toetsing. hierdie verskynsel kan verklaar word as sou die middeloortoestande, wat in die eerste toetsing opgemerk is, ten tye van die tweede toetsing in sommige gevalle alreeds spontaan opgeklaar het^fprescod, 1978). hierdeur word oorverwysing dus verminder. brooks (1976) waarsku ook daarteen dat te veel waarde aan 'n enkele abnormale timpanometriese uitslag geheg word en beklemtoon ook die groot mate van spontane herstel wat voorkom. uit die resultate blyk dit verder dat die leerlinge in skool 1 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die aard en voorkoms van middeloorpatalogiee in laerskole vir normaalhorende blanke kinders 33 in 'n geringe mate swakker gevaar het as die leerlinge in skool 2 op beide siftingstoetse. hierdie verskille is egter nie statisties betekenisvol nie (z = 0,8 < 2,52 maar > 2,57; ζ = 1,66 < 2,57 maar > 2,57). hierdie tendens is moeilik verklaarbaar aangesien daar verwag is dat leerlinge vanuit die hoer sosio-ekonomiese groep minder middeloorprobleme sou vertoon as leerlinge vanuit die laer sosio-ekonomiese groep. redes hiervoor kan wees dat daar miskien nie so 'n groot verskil in sosio-ekonomiese status onder blankes in pretoria is nie (badenhorst, 1987). verder beskik albei skole oor sowel skoolmediese dienste as oudiologiese dienste. ten spyte van die relatief lae sosio-ekonomiese gebied waarin die proefpersone van skool 2 woonagtig is, is dit so dat heelwat van die inwoners by ,groot industriee werksaam is wat goeie mediese fasiliteite bied. 'n laaste faktor wat die resultate kon be'invloed, is die ouderdomme van die kinders. leerlinge in skool 1 was effens jonger as die in skool 2. aangesien jonger kinders 'n groter voorkoms van middeloorpatologie vertoon, kon dit duidelik die rede wees hoekom die resultate van die twee skole nie beduidend van mekaar verskil nie. ten einde te bepaal of die proefpersone wat met die siftingstoetse uitgeval het, wel oor die patologiese middeloorfunksie beskik, is die resultate van die otologiese ondersoeke in verband gebring met die van die opvolgimmittansiemetings. resultate kan soos volg getabelleer word: tabel 3: die otologiese diagnose van die ore wat met die opvolgimmittansiemetings uitval opvolgsifting otologiese diagnose faal patologies normaal skool 1 ,n = 29 / 20/29 (69%) 11/29 (37,9%) 9/29 (31%) skool 2 ν = 24 19/24 (79,1%) 6/24 (25%) 13/24 (54,2%) totaal: (opvolgsifting) ν = 53 ! 39/53 (73,6%) ί 17/53 (32,1%) 22/53 (41,5%) totaal: (uit totale populasie) ν = 200 i 39/100 (19,5%) 17/200 (8,5%) 22/200 (11%) in tabel 3 word die ore wat met die tweede sifting faal, se otologiese diagnose uiteengesit. uit tabel 3 blyk dit dat 'n totaal van 39 uit 53 ore met die tweede sifting uitval (73,6%). van die 53 ore is slegs 17 as patologies geklassifiseer (32,1%). daar kan dus gese word dat 'n totaal van 17 ore, vanuit die aanvanklik getoetste populasie (8,5%), beide die opvolgtoetsing en otologiese diagnose gefaal het. wanneer hierdie persentasie vergelyk word met ander populasies van 'n gemiddelde sosio-ekonomiese status, is dit duidelik dat hier 'n hoe voorkoms van middeloorprobleme is. die volgende voorkomssyfers van otitis media is hier tersaaklik: 2% (amerika), 4,1% (rusland) en 1% (denemarke) (hinchcliffe in northern en downs, 1984). wanneer dit egter vergelyk word met die voorkoms onder hoerisikogroepe, byvoorbeeld indo-chinese vlugtelingkinders (68,8%), is die voorkoms laag (corth en harris, 1984). dit wil dus voorkom asof die voorkoms van middeloorprobleme by die groep graad 1 en 2 leerlinge van pretoria, hoer is as in ander gemiddelde sosio-ekonomiese status populasies, maar laer is as die insidensie in hoerisikogroepe. wanneer die voorkomssyfers van middeloorprobleme by skool 1 (11%) en by skool 2 (6%) bestudeer word, blyk dit dat die resultate van skool 2 effens beter is. op die 1% peil van betekenis is hierdie verskil egter nie statisties beduidend nie. die moontlike redes vir die bevinding is reeds genoem. tabel 4 : die otologiese diagnose van die ore wat met die opvoleimmittansiemeting slaag. opvolgsifting otologiese diagnose slaag patologies normaal skool 1 ν = 29 9/29 (31%) 1/29 (3,4%) 8/29 (27,6%) skool 2 ν = 24 5/24 (20,8%) 1/24 (4,2%) 4/24 (16,7%) totaal: (opvolgsifting) ν = 53 14/53 (26,4%) 2/53 (3,8%) 12/53 (22,6%) totaal: (uit totale populasie) ν = 200 14/200 (7%) 2/200 (1%) 12/200 (6%) in tabel 4 word die ore wat die tweede sifting slaag, se otologiese diagnose uiteengesit. uit tabel 4 blyk dit dat 'n totaal van 14 uit 53 ore, die tweede sifting slaag (26,4%). uit die 14 ore, is slegs 2 as patologies geklassifiseer tydens die otologiese ondersoek (14,3%), terwyl 12 ore (85,7%), normaal was. twee van die ore wat wel afwykend was volgens die otologiese diagnose is as normaal geklassifiseer tydens die opvolgsifting. die een oor het 'n herstelde perforasie gehad en die ander oor was atelektaties. dit gebeur met ander woorde relatief selde dat 'n persoon nie op oudiologiese siftingstoetse uitval nie, maar dan wel as patologies geklassifiseer word (1%). tabel 5: otologiese ondersoek timpaniese membraan totaal normaal 181/200 ν = 200 (90,5%) geskend 8/200 ν = 200 (4%) kliniese otitis media 11/200 ν = 200 (5,5%) in tabel 5 word die resultate van die otologiese ondersoek uiteengesit. uit tabel 5 blyk dit dat vanuit 'n aanvanklik getoetste populasie van 200 ore, 4% 'η geskende timpaniese membraan en 5,5% kliniese otitis media het. sou hierdie persentasies vergelyk word met resultate van soortgelyke die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 carin cilliers, deon rossouw, santie meyer en marina hurter studies, wil dit voorkom asof die prevalensie van middeloorprobleme hier relatief laag is. hier kan die studie van johnson (1970) as voorbeeld genoem word. in die betrokke studie is 3000 indiaanse kinders betrek. 'n voorkomssyfer van 7% gevalle met chroniese otitis media en 8% gevalle met tekens van vorige middeloorpatologie is gevind. in teenstelling hiermee toets fiellau-nikolajsen (1979) kaukasiese kinders en vind 'n baie lae voorkoms van 0,8% defekte van die timpaniese membraan. dit wil dus voorkom asof die proefpersone in hierdie studie, in vergelyking met 'n hoerisikogroep kinders, nie 'n abnormale hoe voorkoms van middeloorprobleme toon nie. tabel 6: sensitiwiteit en spesiwiteit: siftingsimmittansieresultate en otoskopiese ondersoek siftingstoets patologies normaal totaal positief negatief totaal 17 (a) 2 (c) 19 (a + c) 22 (b) 12 (d) 34 (b + d) 39 14 53 (a + b + c + d) sensitiwiteit: a/(a + c) 89,5% ' spesiwiteit: d/(b + d) (frankenburg in northern en downs, 1984) 35,3% in tabel 6 word die sensitiwiteit en spesiwiteit van die immittansiesiftingstoets bereken. dit is dus duidelik dat hoewel die sensitiwiteit hoog is (89,5%), is die spesiwiteit te laag (35,3%): die vraag ontstaan nou of die spesiwiteit van die immittansiesiftingstoets nie sal verbeter indien die akoestiese refleks nie as siftingskriterium gebruik is nie. die rede hiervoor is omdat so 'n groot persentasie kinders ten spyte van 'n tipe a-timpanogram, 'n afwesige akoestiese refleks toon en dus die siftingstoetsing faal. in so 'n geval kan die resultate soos volg getabelleer word: ' tabel 7: sensitiwiteit en spesiwiteit: timpanogramme en otoskopiese ondersoek siftingstoets patologies normaal totaal positief negatief totaal 14 (a) 2 (c) 16 (a + c) 16 (b) 12 (d) 28 (d + b) 30 14 44 (a + b + c + d) sensitiwiteit: a/(a + c) 87,5% spesiwiteit: d/(b + d) (frankenburg in northern en downs, 1984) 42,9% in tabel 7 word die sensitiwiteit en spesiwiteit van die immittansiesiftingstoets bereken waar reflekse nie as maatstaf gebruik word nie. die afleiding kan dus gemaak word dat die spesiwiteit van die immittansiesiftingstoets wel van 35,3 •ria 42,9 verbeter het, maar dat dit steeds baie laag is. dit is nogtans bemoedigend dat kinders wat middeloorprobleme toon selde nie deur middel van immittansiesiftingstoetse geidentifiseer kan word nie. gevolgtrekkings in teenstelling met die algemene opvatting dat daar 'n hoer voorkoms van middeloorprobleme in 'n laer sosio-ekonomiese gebied as in 'n hoer sosio-ekonomiese gebied sal wees (klein, 1978) het die studie dit as onwaar bewys. feitlik ewe veel leerlinge uit beide die hoe en lae sosio-ekonomiese omgewing faal die eerste, sowel as die tweede siftingsprosedure. redes hiervoor kan onder andere klein verskille in sosio-ekonomiese status onder blankes, goeie mediese en oudiologiese dienste ook in die laer sosio-ekonomiese gebied en die effens jonger ouderdomme van leerlinge in skool 2 wees. ten opsigte van die voorkoms van middeloordisfunksie (oudiometries bepaal) faal 'n totaal van 19,5% die eerste, sowel as die tweede oudiologiese sifting. word dit vergelyk met resultate van soortgelyke studies, wil dit voorkom asof die prevalensie van middeloordisfunksie by die 2 groepe kinders nie kommerwekkend hoog is nie. redes hiervoor kan die gereelde skoolmediese, sowel as oudiologiese dienste waaroor hierdie skole beskik, wees, 'n totaal van 17 ore (8,5%), vanuit die aanvanklik getoetste populasie, faal beide die opvolgtoetsing en die otologiese diagnose. verder was daar geen statisties beduidende verskille tussen die twee groepe persone nie. ten opsigte van die aard van die middeloorpatologie het 4% 'n geskende timpaniese membraan en het 5,5% kliniese otitis media. in vergelyking met ander studies wil dit voorkom asof hierdie proefpersone in vergelyking met hoerisikogroepe nie 'n hoe voorkoms van middeloorprobleme vertoon nie (johnson, 1970): dit blyk egter dat die voorkoms wel hoer is as ander kaukasiese populasies (fiellau-nikolajsen, 1^79). ten opsigte van 'n vergelyking tussen die mediese en oudiologiese resultate, blyk dit dat die sensitiwiteit van die immittansiemetings goed is, maar die spesiwiteit baie laag is (35,3%). indien akoestiese reflekse nie as siftingskriterium gebruik word nie, verbeter die spesiwiteit na 42,9%. hierdie studie verskaf belangrike inligting aangesien dit die eeitste keer is wat so 'n studie onder kaukasiers in suidafrika uitgevoer is. die voorkomsfrekwensie van 9,5% vir middeloordisfunksie soos in die studie verkry is, sal waarskynlik wissel in ander populasies, geografiese areas, ouderdomsgroepe, ensovoorts, maar 'n prevalensiesyfer vir 'n groep graad 1 en 2 blanke kinders is nou beskikbaar. ! verwysings i badenhorst, m.s. persoonlike onderhoud. departement stadsen streekbeplanning, universiteit van pretoria, 1987. barrett, k.a. hearing and immittance screening of school-age children. in katz, j. (red.) handbook of clinical audiology. baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. brooks, c.n. school screening for middle ear effusions. the annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology, suppl. 25(2), 223—2238 1976. corth, s.b. & r.w. harris. incidence of middle ear disease in indochinese refugee schoolchildren. audiology, 23, 27—37 1984. feldman, a.s. & l.a. wilber. acoustic impedance and-admittance. baltimore: williams & wilkens, 1977. fiellau-nikolajsen, m. tympanometry ι prediction of the magnitude of hearing loss in _preschool-children with secretory otitis media. scandinavian audiology, supplementum 17 6 8 7 2 , 1979. . / hugo, r. normatiewe evaluasie van otitis media as sekondere patologie. suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir opvoedkunde 5 8 3 8 8 , 1984. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die aard en voorkoms van middeloorpatalogiee in laerskole vir normaalhorende blanke kinders 35 jerger, j. clinical experience with impedance audiometry. archives of otolaryngology, 92, pp. 3 1 1 3 2 4 , 1970. johnson, r.l. a abbreviated impedance bridge technique for school screening. in feldman, a.s. & l.a. wilber (reds.) acoustic impedance and admittance. baltimore: williams and wilkens, 1970. klein, j.o. epidemiology of otitis media. in harford, e.r., f.h. bess, c.d. bluestone, j.o. klein (reds.): impedance screening of middle ear disease in children. new york: grune & stratton, 1978. lewis, n. otitis media and linguistic incompetence. archives of otolaryngology, 192, 3 8 7 3 9 0 , 1976. mcwilliams, b.j., h.l. morris & r.l. shelton. cleft palate speech. st. louis: the cv mosby company, 1984. miller, p. incidence and time course of otitis media in children. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 audiology in practice, 1(3), 1, 1985. northern j.l. & m.p. downs. hearing in children. baltimore: the williams & wilkens company, 1984. prescod, s.v. audiological handbook of hearing disorders. new york: litten education publishing inc., 1978. smit, g.j. navorsingsmetodes in die gedragswetenskappe. pretoria: haum opvoedkundige uitgewers, 1983. van berge, m.m. die sosio-ekonomiese status van woongebiede in pretoria. ongepubliseerde skripsie, universiteit van pretoria, 1982. voogt, g.r., halama, a.r. & van der merwe, c.a. immittance screening in black preschool children attending day-care centres. audiology, 25, 158—164, 1986. wilson, j. deafness in developing countries. archives of otolaryngology, 3(1), pp. 2 9 , 1985. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) new a m o m e n t of truth from amtronix οτι co ν 152 canal hearing a i d available in standard (modular) and custom made. the 152 comes standard with three audiological controls as shown in fig. a: a-gram control = low frequency adjustment (green arrow and dots). db hl control = output/gain adjustment (red arrow and dots). feedback control (fc) = high frequency adjustment (orange arrow and dots). volume control button (with o n / o f f fig. β shows minimum & maximum of function) red dot: right version. controls. blue dot: left version. battery compartment (below lid). sound outlet. vent opening (in custom version). state of the art technology for the year 1990 available from amtronix in 1988. the almost impossible made possible, thanks to oticon! 1. 2. 3. 4. computerized insertion gain optimizer features: * the sure way to the right results. * read in personal patient and audiogram data. * measure open ear and occluded ear gain. compare insertion gain curves and select hearing aid. * programme alternate test procedures; if needed. * get it right first time. the igo 1000 system offers you a compact, easy-to-use diagnostic and treatment package. * complete test capability. high resolution, full colour monitor and printouts. * high speed and extreme test accuracy. * ' efficient, easy-access storage of patient data. a modern, ergonomic design for maximum user comfort. diagnostic a u d i o m e t r y the friendly midimate 602 diagnostic audiometer for audiologists demanding capability, simplicity and efficiency. features: * brief automatic check at start-up. * fast menu-driven setup of your own test. * ; \ p r e p r ° g r a ; ™ a b l e t e s t s e t u p s f o r instant one-key retrieval of your favourite setups saves you time and trouble in the daily routine. ^ simple and logical multilingual menu-driven display dialogue makes the midimate 602 easy to learn and easy to use. ' transflective blacklighted display is easily readable under any lighting conditions while supertwist technology enlarges the viewing angle. / few operator controls all logically located on front panel for optimal operation convenience and accuracy. a m t r o n i x (pty) ltd., p.o. box 630, bedfordview 2008 phone (011) 6221743 telex 4-50033 amtron fax 6221306 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 the case for cognitive neuropsychological remediation marlene behrmann, ma (speech path.) (witwatersrand) sharon herdan, ba (sp. and h. therapy) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract developments in the field of cognitive neuropsychology have recently begun to have an impact on therapeutic approaches to aphasia. increasingly, clinicians have started adopting theoretical models of normal cognitive processing for the assessment and rehabilitation of individuals with acquired language deficits. this study describes and evaluates a single case, tested and treated within this framework. the subject, a surface dysgraphic, was assessed in detail pre-therapy and a deficit in lexical processing was diagnosed. treatment designed to stimulate the lexical route of writing was undertaken and post-therapy evaluation was conducted. the results revealed a significant improvement in the writing of both regular and irregular treated words with generalisation to certain untreated words. such improvement was shown to be a direct consequence of the intervention procedure. therapeutic findings strengthen the basis of cognitive processing models which, in turn, provide a streamlined clinical framework for the practitioner. opsomming resente ontwikkeling in die vakgebied van kognitiewe neuropsigologie begin om 'n invloed uit te oefen op terapeutiese benaderings tot afhsie. terapeute begin toenemend om teoretiese modelle van normale kognitiewe prosessering aan te wend vir die evaluering en rehabilitering van individue met verworwe taalafwykings. hierdie studie beskryfen evalueer 'n enkele geval wat binne hierdie raamwerk geevalueer en behandel is. voor die aanvang van die behandeling is 'n diepte-evaluasie op 'n pasient met oppervlakdisgrafie uitgevoer en 'n afwyking in leksikale prosessering is gediagnoseer. behandeling gerig op die stimulering van die leksikale roete van skryfvermoens is uitgevoer en die pasient is na afloop van die behandeling geherevalueer. resultate dui op 'n beduidende verbetering in die skryfvan beide reelmatige en onreelmatige behandelde woorde met veralgemening na sekere onbehandelde woorde. hierdie verbetering is bewys om 'n direkte gevolg van die intervensieprosedure te wees. terapeutiese bevindinge verstrek die basis van kognitiewe prosesseringsmodelle wat op hulle beurt 'n gerasionaliseerde kliniese raamwerk aan die klinikus verskaf. the'last decade has witnessed the rapid expansion of a discipline which has come to be called cognitive neuropsychology. this discipline postulates that an information-processing system, consisting of a number of modular subcomponents, underlies the normal execution of any cognitive activity (coltheart 1983, 1985).!these modular, domain-specific subcomponents function independently and are interconnected by a network of pathways. whereas all the subsystems function adequately in normals, they are susceptible to discrete disruption following brain damage. brain damage, thus, leads to observable dissociations between functions. such dissociations have been studied in detail and have evolved into a detailed description of the various patterns of acquired cognitive deficits (see coltheart 1980). the study of brain damaged patients within the cognitive neuropsychological paradigm has been informative on a number of levels. it has confirmed the existence of theoretically postulated dissociations and has lent credibility to the existing theoretical models, especially in the field of acquired dyslexia. a further benefit of such an approach has been in the clinical sphere. sophisticated diagnostic methods have evolved in order to assess the independent functioning of the modular subcomponents. additionally, and more recently, remediation programs designed to restore the malfunctioning subsystem, have been described in the literature (behrmann 1987; byng and coltheart 1986 and de partz 1986). the cognitive neuropsychological approach has facilitated the locating of a specific acquired deficit in precise terms and has then provided guidelines for formulating an individually tailored treatment program. according to hatfield (1983), this new orientation has made it possible to develop new methods which depend on a wellthought out theory of normal cognitive processing. a basic concern of the above cited remediation studies is methodological integrity. treatment studies have always been subject to methodological inadequacies and as such, results have been ambiguous and undefined. in many instances, it has been impossible to determine whether the positive findings were attributable to the treatment itself or to other co-occurring but non-specific causes such as spontaneous recovery or increased motivation (byng and coltheart 1986). the cognitive neuropsychological remediation studies, on the other hand, have been designed in such a way that the outcome of therapy may be unequivocally interpreted and as such, satisfy rigorous methodological criteria. the purpose of this paper is to describe a remediation study based on the cognitive neuropsychological theory of writing. in recent years, models of the normal writing process have become increasingly well developed and detailed investigations of the acquired dysgraphias have been underdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 © sasha 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 marlene behrmann and sharon herdan taken (margolin 1984; patterson and shewell 1987). despite the fact that such theoretical development has much potential for therapeutic application, there are remarkably few treatment studies in this area. the present study was designed to apply a model of normal writing to the remediation of an acquired agraphia. as in most current models of the normal writing process, the writing model adopted for this study includes two major routes (see figure 1). the phonological route involves the segmental translation from phonology to orthography spelling of words directly from the lexicon and are thus forced to rely on the phonological route whereby phonological segments are translated into orthographic representations (ellis 1984; hatfield and patterson 1983). the outcome of this is that irregular and homophonic spelling is adversely affected and since phoneme-grapheme conversion is used, resulting spelling errors are phonologically plausible for example, 'yacht' may be written 'yot'. previous studies (beauvois and derouesne 1981; hatfield auditory analysis w phonological non-lexical route 1 γ w lexical route r phonological representation: word recognition ι figure 1: simplified information processing model of two major routes for single word writing to dictation (after ellis 1984: patterson 1980.) through the application of rules of phoneme-to-graphemeconversion. this route may only be utilised to write regular words whose spelling conforms to the spelling conventions of the language. in addition, one may use this route for assembling the spelling of unfamiliar words or non-words which do not have prior entries in the lexicon. the second major route, the lexical route, involves an association between word spelling and its meaning. thus, the writer must recognise the phonological input of the dictated word and must access its semantic representation and orthographic form. this may only be achieved with words which have prior entries in the lexicon, i.e., are real words which have been encountered previously. the use of the lexical procedure is critical for spelling irregular words (which cannot be assembled through phoneme-grapheme conversion) and homophonic words (which are phonologically identical but differ in orthographic representation and semantics) (hatfield and patterson 1983; margolin 1984). surface dysgraphia is one of the patterns of deficit identified on the basis of models such as the one above. this problem, alternatively termed lexical or orthographic dysgraphia, arises from the defective functioriing of the lexical route. patients with this disorder have lost the ability to access the and patterson 1983) have focussed on the assessment and classification of acquired surface dysgraphia. beauvois and derouesne (1981), for example, reported that their subject; r.g. wrote almost all non-words correctly but that his real word spelling depended on the degree of orthographic ambi-, guity between sound and print. as words became less regular in their spelling pattern, so his performance dete-j riorated. hatfield and patterson's (1983) subject, t.p. alsoj spelled via the non-lexical routine. he too showed superior/ performance on regular compared with irregular words and many of his errors were phonologically plausible (e.g., laugh — laf). treatment of this disorder was undertaken by hatfield (1983) who described a positive therapy outcome for her surface dysgraphic subject. another study of surface dysgraphia treatment, undertaken by behrmann (1987), was devoted to the retraining of the subject's lexical route through the use of homophonic rather than irregular words. the subject benefitted from this intervention, showing marked improvement on treated homophones (from 49% to 67%). in addition, the subject improved on writing untreated irregular words (32% to 67% across two lists), implying that a broad, rather than a specific, change within lexical processing had been implemented. minimal improvement on untreated homophones was noted, suggesting that the the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the case of cognitive neuropsychological remediation 5 lexical route was not entirely restored or alternatively, word-specific learning is necessary for homophones. the subject of the present study, ccm, participated in the behrmann (1987) study and as is evident, did not achieve perfect scores on irregular word writing. she has subsequently participated in a second phase of the treatment program and the results of this phase are reported here. aims the aim of the present study was to investigate the efficacy of a treatment program conducted within a cognitive neuropsychological framework, on a subject with acquired dysgraphia. more specifically, this study was designed to investigate the subject's present use of the lexical procedure in writing to dictation, and to implement a therapy procedure to further enhance the use of the lexical route in writing. subject description a. case history ccm suffered a cerebro-vascular accident (cva) in june 1984, at the age of 53 years. a ct scan suggested that the cva involved the middle cerebral artery in the left temporoparietal region. clinically, no hemiplegia nor hemianopia were noted. ccm had a history of hypertension for which she was receiving medication at the time of the stroke. ccm, a high-school educated woman, was employed as a secretary pre-morbidly. although she is bilingual (english and afrikaans), she has resided in an english speaking environment for a long time and uses english almost exclusively. b. aphasia testing the western aphasia battery (kertesz 1980) was administered in may 1985 and revealed an aphasia quotient of 73 (cut-off point 93,8) and a pattern of deficits resembling conduction aphasia. her spontaneous speech was fluent with a mean length of utterance of 6,8 and a marked breakdown in repetition was observed, j ' the bishop and byng (1984) lexical understanding with visual and semantic distractors, in which ccm was required to match a printed! word with the correct picture, was administered. distractor pictures included a closely related semantic picture, a distant semantic distractor, a picture which is visually similar to the target and an unrelated picture. ccm's score of 39/40 was indicative of her preserved single word level semantic ability. relatively well preserved syntactic abilities were also observed as ccm scored 85% on the test of receptive grammar (trog) (bishop 1982). c. test of reading and writing: detailed testing of ccm's reading and writing have been described in the previous study (behrmann 1987). in sum, ccm demonstrated remarkably well-preserved reading skills at the single word level. she scored 100% on a lexical decision task in which she had to decide whether a string of letters constituted and english word or not. in addition, various word lists were given to her for oral reading. reading performance was good even on irregular words (mean 96%) which utilise the lexical route. similarly, her ability to use the non-lexical route (segmental translation from orthography to phonology) was good as she read 83% non-words correctly. neither imageability nor frequency played a significant role in her reading performance. ccm's superior reading ability was in marked contrast to her impaired writing. only on non-word writing did ccm perform relatively well (80%), suggesting the selective preservation of the non-lexical phonological writing route. results of several other writing tests (see table 1) revealed a significant difference in her ability to write regular compared with irregular words (bub and kertesz list, fisher exact test p<0.05). this result was not borne out on the roeltgen and heilman (1984) list (fisher exact test ρ = .21) possibly because this latter list is made up of low frequency words, and as such, it presents a potentially confounding interaction of variables. ccm scored poorly on the regular words of this list too, indicating a possible floor effect of this list. table 1: results of ccm's writing performance across various word lists test bub and kertesz (1982) regular words irregular words writing score 35/40 (88%) 15/30 (50%) roeltgen and heilman (1984) regular words irregular words 27/45 (60%) 23/45 (51%) homophone writing 93/138 (67%) non-word writing 24/30 (80%) ccm also performed poorly on homophone writing to dictation (67%). this task obligatorily requires the use of the lexical procedure in order to translate the ambiguous phonological input (for example /seil/) into the correct orthographic form (either 'sale' or 'sail' depending on intended meaning and context). the regular-irregular word discrepancy, together with her poor homophone writing suggested an impairment in the use of the lexical procedure for writing. this is further borne out by an analysis of the types of errors made by ccm. such an analysis revealed that writing was accomplished via the preserved phonological, non-lexical route for writing. most of her writing errors involved phonological conversion, for example tomb—• "toom", ocean —•"owshin", effort—• "evort", ritual—• "richell". certain errors suggested that ccm was using afrikaans rules of phoneme-grapheme conversion in her writing of english words. examples of this include contract—•"kontrak" and crime—•"krime". additionally, /w/ and ivl were confused, for example victim—•"wictim". these confusions are not surprising since ccm grew up in a bilingual community and was a fluent afrikaans reader and writer. in sum, the results of ccm's reading and writing testing indicates that orthographic knowledge for word recognition in reading is separate from the orthographic knowledge necessary for correct spelling in writing. while her reading was remarkably preserved, she displayed all the symptoms typidie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 marlene behrmann and sharon herdan cally associated with surface dysgraphia that is, a reliance on the phonological route in writing, a discrepancy between performance on regular and irregular words and impaired homophone writing. baseline measurement prior to instituting therapy, an adequate baseline measurement must be taken to serve as the basis for comparison of outcome and the evaluation of effectiveness of intervention (mcreynolds and kearns 1983). baseline stability and consistency are critical features which, when observed, rule out maturational and spontaneous recovery effects (vetter 1985). as therapy was to be directed to the restoration of the lexical route through the teaching of irregular words, further spelling assessment was carried out. this assessment was loosely based on the format used by byng and coltheart (1986) in the evaluation of their surface dyslexic patient. four hundred and fifty most frequent words, taken from francis and kucera (1982), constituted the baseline measure. owing to the possible confounding effect of frequency, words were divided into three frequency bandwidths for testing: (i) word 1-150 : frequency 69975 to 580 per hundred thousand (ii) word 151-300 : frequency 574 to 330 per hundred thousand (iii) word 301-450 : frequency 329 to 231 per hundred thousand. testing took place over three sessions during which fifty words from each bandwidth were assessed. overall, ccm wrote 359/450 (80%) words correctly. results across sessions were not statistically significant (chi squared = 1,43, ρ >0,5) and hence, stability of performance was observed (session 1 115/150 (77%); session 2 121/150 (81%); session 3 123/150 (82%)). a significant difference (chi square = 8,3, ρ <0,01) across frequency bands was noted with ccm scoring 87% for the most frequent items, 77% for the second group and 75% for the last frequent items. eighty six of the 91 incorrectly written words formed the basis of the therapy. the five words not selected comprised four words that had occurred more than once in the list and one americanised word which was felt not to be in ccm's repertoire. t h e r a p y ( a 1 ) (one week) selection of words (a and b) t p r e t h e r a p y measure ( t w o weeks : 3 baseline measures) treatment procedure the aims of treatment were a. to enhance the use of the lexical procedure in writing, through teaching ccm a group of irregular words, b. to ascertain whether the improvement would generalise to a set of untreated irregular words. c. to establish that the change (if any) that took place was a direct consequence of treatment. design in order to achieve the final aim, a multiple baseline design was used. whereas several functions were assessed pretherapy (semantics, syntax and writing), only one function was to be treated. if improvement was a specific consequence of the treatment program, only the treated function should improve. if, however, improvement resulted from a generalised change such as increased motivation or spontaneous recovery, all linguistic functions should be affected. in order to assess the generalisation of treatment, a crossover design of material was employed. this required that one set of words (a) be taught prior to a second set (b) so that one may evaluate the effect of therapy on β while a is taught and vice versa. to this end, the 86 incorrect words were randomly divided up into two groups, one of 44 words and one of 42 words. these two groups were matched for frequency. each group was further subdivided (a1 and a2, b1 and b2) so that shorter word lists could be taught during therapy. treatment took place for two one-hour sessions per week. each list of words was taught for two consecutive sessions and intra-therapy assessment was conducted at the end of two weeks when the entire group a had been taught. posttherapy evaluation was undertaken after both sets a and β had been covered. figure 2 below illustrates the methodology as well as the timing of treatment. i therapy (a2) 1 (one week) i two weeks i n t r a t h e r a p y measures i post-therapy measure ( t w o weeks : three baseline measures) ί t h e r a p y ( b 2 ) ~ « t h e r a p y ( b 1 ) (one week) ( 0 n e week) figure 2: methodological design and timing of ccm's treatment plan the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the case of cognitive neuropsychological remediation several critical issues were considered in devising specific therapy strategies. in order to retain word-specific representations, a holistic approach had to be adopted and therapy had to be directed to maintaining the link between semantics and the visual form of the word. this meant that the focus of therapy would be on each individual word as a whole item and sub-lexical or part-word processing would be de-emphasized. as ccm was surface dysgraphic but not surface dyslexic, tasks which relied on reading were limited so that the focus of therapy was on the writing, and not on the reading process. different exercises were employed in the initial and subsequent sessions. the first session involved showing ccm the target word and linking it to its meaning through the use of a dictionary. in those cases where it was possible, the word was represented pictorially. following this, ccm was required to copy the word out several times and then to spell it aloud orally so that practice in several modalities would strengthen the link between the word and its spelling. thereafter, she wrote the word to dictation. home practice was undertaken after each session and involved additional copying and picture-word matching tasks. the aim of these tasks was essentially to link the orthography to the semantics and to aid the memorising of the written form of the word. the second session involved correction and revision of the home exercises followed by a series of multiple choice tasks in which the subject selected the correct written form from several options. she then wrote the words to dictation and incorrect spellings were highlighted with coloured pens. finally, ccm was required to write the words to dictation. homework was given again. treatment of groups a2, b1 and b2 followed the same format as outlined for group al. practice at home was restricted to those words which were being taught at that particular phase of the program. post-therapy testing was conducted after a five day break from treatment. the post-therapy baseline testing included the re-administration of all words in lists a and β as well as all words written correctly at pre-therapy testing. the order of testing was the same as that of the pre-therapy assessment. results a comparison of ccm's performance preand post-therapy revealed a significant improvement in her spelling ability following intervention (cochran q test x2 = 38.7, ρ >0,001). table 2 illustrates the scores obtained during the preand post-therapy assessments. there was no significant difference in ccm's three post-therapy measures, indicating a stability in her performance across time (x2 = 0.99, p<0.5). the observed improvement was equally distributed across the three frequency bandwidths (see table 3), suggesting a broad rather than a frequency-specific effect of treatment. an evaluation of pre-, intraand post-therapy measures suggested that the final improvement noted was directly attributable to intervention. as can be seen from table 4. ccm obtained 91% correct spelling on the treated group a words at the intra-therapy measure compared with the pretherapy score (mcnemar's test x2 = 38, p>0.001). the untreated group β words were also markedly improved (60% correct) compared with the pre-therapy score (mcnemar's test x2 = 23, ρ >0.001). this suggests that a certain amount of generalisation had taken place from group a to group β at the intra-therapy stage. this was a pleasing, although not totally surprising result. specific carry-over from group a to group β words was seen on certain words, for example, she learned to spell 'which' in group a and generalised the 'wh' spelling to 'where' and 'while' in table 2: results obtained on baseline measures at pre-and post-therapy levels baseline 1 baseline 2 baseline 3 pre-therapy 115/150 (77%) 121/150 (81%) 123/150 (82%) post-therapy j 143/150 (95%) 139/150 (93%) 140/150 (93%) table 3: results obtained within frequency bandwidths at preand post-therapy testing words 1-50 words 151-300 words 301-450 pre-therapy 131/150 (85%) 116/150 (77%) 112/150 (75%) post-therapy 146/150 (97%) 143/150 (95%) 133/150 (87%) table 4: results of group a and group β words at pre-intra and post-therapy testing pre-therapy intra-therapy post-therapy group a words 0/44 (0%) 40/44 (91%) 41/44 (93%) group β words 0/44 (0%) 25/42 (60%) 35/42 (83%) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34,1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) marlene behrmann and sharon herdan group b. similarly, the correct spelling of 'effort' learned on group a was generalised to the correct spelling of effect in group β the generalisation from group a to group β cannot be totally accounted for on this basis, however, and it is s u g g e s t e d that even at this intra-therapy stage, the lexical route had been activated and was being utilised for writing untreated words. post-therapy testing on the now (treated group β words also revealed that further gains were made when spcific attention was paid to this group and a posttherapy gain of 23% over the intra-therapy score was noted in the group β score. there was no significant difference between group a intratherapy and post-therapy scores (mcnemar's text x2 = 0, ρ <0,5), indicating that ccm was able to retain the words learned even when no practice was allowed. in sum, the post-therapy results were encouraging on several counts: intra-therapy generalization was observed; further benefit was later noted on group b; and there was no decay on group a indicating some longer-term effect. however, in order to ascertain whether ccm had learned something specific about the treated words or whether she had gained a more general ability to spell any set of words, the bub and kertesz (1982) word list was re-administered. the results revealed a significant improvement in the writing of the irregular bub and kertesz words (mcnemar's test x2 = 3,3, ρ<0.01) and some additional improvement on the regular words from 35/40 (88%) to 38/40 (95%). this finding strongly suggests that the lexical route is functional and is processing words that have not been individually treated. in order to assess whether the observed change in writing was directly attributable to therapy and not simply a function of a non-specific variable, ccm's sentence comprehension was re-evaluated. since sentence comprehension bears no theoretical relationship to writing, no improvement in this area is expected. indeed, if improvement were noted, ccm's changes in writing ability might have been a result of diffuse variables rather than intervention per se. preand post-therapy scores on the test of receptive grammar were identical (85%), confirming that the intervention procedure had a specific effect on writing alone. likewise, no change in ccm's digit span was noted post-therapy, a finding which further confirms the specific effects of therapy. discussion the results of this study revealed stable post-treatment performance on writing (directly attributable to the intervention program) and statistically significant improvement on treated words with generalisation to untreated words. these findings suggest that it is possible to retrain writing through a whole-word technique in which the semantic and orthographic links are strengthened. the adoption of a model-based approach allowed the clinician to clearly define the nature of the underlying spelling deficit and then to -develop a remediation program suited to treating the disorder. the fact that the group β words showed some improvement in intra-therapy testing implies that the treatment of group a words did not merely act as a rote learning of a list of words. instead, it suggests that a more general spelling ability had been activated. it has been previously suggested that complete restoration of lexical processing may only be achieved through wordspecific attention and that the spread of generalization (behrman 1987) is minimal. this has largely been refuted in the present case since improved lexical processing was noted on group β words prior to its undergoing treatment and change on untreated irregular words (bub and kertesz list) was obtained. although the type of treatment program adopted in this study does not differ dramatically from traditional approaches to treatment in practical terms, it diverges in terms of its theoretical formulation. the present treatment approach is motivated by a fine-grained theoretical analysis of the subject's presenting deficits. it adopts a model of normal cognitive processing as the theoretical basis for isolating and identifying the locus of functional breakdown and for evolving a compatible treatment plan. unlike traditional forms of therapy, the present approach provides explicit pointers for remediating the underlying cause of the deficit. traditional forms of writing therapy do not always provide the same amount of certainty and specificity. they merely alert the therapist to the general direction which treatment should take (perkins 1985). therapy studies such as the one outlined here have an additional benefit. their results feed back into the theoretical model and either, if correct, confirm the practicality of the theory or, if incorrect, demand a modification of the model. to this end, therapy is dependent on theory and theory is enhanced and altered by therapy. it is suggested that this symbiotic relationship has much to offer clinicians working with brain-damaged subjects. although the therapy efficacy hypothesis was put to the test in a single subject and the results restricted in generalizability, the data contributes to the cumulative evaluation of the models used. further case replications are imperative, however, and further developments in this field are critically dependent on the widespread application of this therapeutic approach. references beauvois, m.f. and derouesne, j. (1981) lexical or orthographic agraphia. brain, 104, 21-49. i behrmann, m. (1987) the rites of righting writing: homophone remediation in surface dysgraphia. cognitive neuropsychology (in press). | bishop, d. (1982) trog: test of receptive grammar abingdon, oxon : thomas leach (for medical research council). | bishop, d. and byng, s. (1984) assessing semantic comprehension: methodological considerations and a new critical test. cognitive neuropsychology, 1, 3, 233-243. ι bub, d. and kertesz, a. (1982) deep agraphia. brain and language, 17, 146-165. byng, s. and coltheart, m. (1986) aphasia therapy research : methodological requirements and illustrative results. in e. hjelmquist and l.b. nilsson (eds.) communication and handicap. aspects of psychological compensation and technical aids. amsterdam : north holland publishing co., 1986. coltheart, m. (1980) varieties of acquired dysgraphia. unpublished paper, birkbeck college, university of london. coltheart, m. (1983) aphasia therapy research : a single case study approach. in c. code and d.j. muller.jeds.) aphasia therapy. london, edward arnold. coltheart, m. (1985) cognitive neuropsychology and the study of reading. in m.i. posner and o.s.m.-martin (eds.) attention and performance xi. lawrence erlbaum associates, new jersey. de partz, m.p. (1986) re-education of a deep dyslexic patient: rationale of methods and results. cognitive neuropsychology, 3, 149-177. / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the case of cognitive neuropsychological remediation ellis, a.w. (1984) reading, writing and dyslexia : a cognitive analysis. london, lawrence erlbaum associates. francis, w.m. and kucera, h. (1982) frequency analysis of english usage lexicon and grammar. boston, houghton miffin and co. hatfield, f.m. (1983) aspects of acquired dysgraphia and implications for re-education. in c. code and d.j. muller (eds.) aphasia therapy. london: edward arnold. hatfield, f.m. and patterson, k.e. (1983) phonological spelling. quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 35a, 451-468. kertesz, a. (1980) the western aphasia battery. university of western ontario, london, canada. margolin, d. (1984) the neuropsychology of writing and spelling: semantic, phonological, motor and perceptual processes. quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 36a, 459-489. 9 mcreynolds, l.v. and kearns, k.p. (1983) single subject experimental designs in communicative disorders. university park press, baltimore. patterson, k.e. (1986) lexical but not semantic spelling. cognitive neuropsychology, 3, 3, 341-367. patterson, k.e. and shewell, c. (1987) speak and spell : dissociations and word-class effects. in m. coltheart, r. job and g. sartori (eds) cognitive neuropsychology of language. lawrence erlbaum associates ltd, london. perkins, w.h. (1985) from clinical dispenser to clinical scientist. seminars in speech and language, 6, 1, 13-20. roeltgen, d.p. and heilman, k.m. (1984) lexical agraphia. brain, 197, 811-827. vetter, d.k. (1985) evaluation of clinical intervention: accountability. seminars in speech and language, 6, 1, 55-64. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the performance of stutterers on selected central auditory tests gabrielle m. karr β .a. (sp, & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) psychological and guidance services, transvaal education dept., johannesburg summary the possibility of central auditory involvement in stutterers was investigated. five confirmed stutterers and five nonstutterers between the ages of 20 and 26 years served as subjects. the phonetically balanced cid w-22 auditory lists presented with ipsilateral and contralateral white noise broad band masking (at signal to noise ratios of 0 db and l o d b ) a n d the staggered spondaic word test were utilized. no significant differences between the groups were revealed, indicating intact functioning of both groups on these tests. possible reasons for this were postulated. response trends exhibited by all subjects were discussed. opsomming hierdie studie beoog om moontlike sentraal-ouditiewe betrokkenheid in hakkelaars te ondersoek. vyf gevestigde hakkelaars en vyf nie-hakkelaars tussen die ouderdomme van 20 en 26 jaar is as proefpersone gebruik. die foneties gebalanseerde cid w-22 ouditiewe lyste, aangebied met ipsilaterale en kontralaterale wit lawaai bree-band maskering (teen sein t o t ruis verhouding van 0 db en — 10 db), en die verspringende spondee woordtoets (vsw) is benuttig. geen merkbare verskille is tussen die groepe gevind nie."hierdie resultate dui ongeskonde funksionering betreffende hierdie toetse vir altwee groepe aan. moontlike redes hiervoor is gepostuleer. responsneigings van al die proefpersone is bespreek. many hypotheses concerning the etiology of stuttering have been postulated. sander25 summarized the causation areas of stuttering considered in the literature as those of reinforced behaviour, emotion and organicity. the latter model is relevant to the present study. within the framework of the possibility of an organic component in stuttering, a neurological involvement has been suggested. the orton-travis theory of cerebral dominance (1931) postulated that due to a lack of maturation in the cortical speech areas, intercerebral hemisphere conflict occurs cerebral dominance does not develop and stuttering'results.4 progress in testing methods and subsequent physiological findings led to the rejection of this theory. nevertheless an interest in it persists. jones9 using the wada-rasmussen sodium amytol technique, concluded that stutterers do exhibit intercerebral conflict for speech, but controversial findings have been reported. similarly, the performance of stutterers on dichotic tests, (which assess temporal lobe functioning11) is held in question. 9 ' 2 7 • 3 2 in addition, the hypothesis that stuttering is a sub-class of aphasia36 indicates that temporal lobe functioning in stutterers needs to be clarified. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) central auditory testing in stutterers 1 0 1 wolf and wolf8 suggested that a brainstem lesion results in stuttering and the performance of stutterers on the synthetic sentence identification test with an ipsilateral competing message (a test reported to be sensitive to brainstem lesions11 ) was reported to be inferior to that of nonstutterers.31 however, further investigation to support or refute the existence of brainstem lesions in stutterers, is indicated. the auditory cortex consists of the primary and secondary auditory areas. stimulation of the former produces the sensation of hearing non-verbal sounds,22 while stimulation of the latter evokes the sensation of hearing verbal signals17 and thus participate in speech decoding. the secondary auditory cortex, lying in the lateral convex portions of the temporal lobe, is thought to be responsible for the analysis and synthesis of acoustic stimuli and differentiates between simultaneously presented acoustic information.17 the interaction of the higher brainstem and the dominant tempero-parieto-occipital region enables speech comprehension.22 the secondary auditory areas and the post central and premotor articulation areas are connected by u-shaped structures and are within the subcortical gray matter of the cortex, 1 7 thus revealing the reality of neurological connections between the speech and auditory systems. a central disorder has been defined as an . . . impairment of the cerebral cortex and subcortical areas, probably to the level of the brainstem14 so that primary sites of a central auditory system (cas) lesion may be considered to be in the brainstem or in heschl's gyrus.11 this study is concerned with the functioning of these two areas in stutterers. lesions at these sites do not lead to impaired scores on conventional pure-tone or speech audiometric tests.5 · 1 1 the use of speech has been favoured in cas testing as the functions of discrimination and integration and the participation of the language processess are tapped.5 the probability rules of redundancy and predictability involved in speech perception had to be eliminated and thus the speech material utilized in cas tests had to be suitably selected. tests such as the speech with alternating masking index (swami)3s and fusion testss have been utilized in brain stem lesion testing and jerger11 suggests that individuals with brainstem involvement have greater difficulty with a complex monaural task than a dichotic task. the use of ipsilateral masking has been suggested as a factor which increases the complexity of speech identification tasks.5 dichotic tests have been used in evaluating temporal lobe functioning.35 a dichotic task may be viewed as one involving simultaneous sets of information being presented to each ear. the staggered spondaic word test (ssw) devised by k a t z 1 3 is partially dichotic and is reportedly a cas test sensitive to temporal lobe lesions.6 the performance of normals on dichotic tasks reveal superior right ear performance which is known as the right ear affect (rea), and reduced scores in the ear contralateral to a cas lesion have been reported on dichotic tasks.9 the ssw yields right and left ear scores and it may therefore reveal differences in ear performance in stutterers as compared to nonstutterers. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 0 2 gabrielle μ. karr methodology it was hypothesized that the performance of stutterers on a brainstem and/or temporal lobe cas test would be inferior to that of nonstutterers. inferior performance by the stutterers will be reflected by decreased ear difference scores as compared to the scores of the nonstutterers. s u b j e c t s . two groups of subjects (ss) were selected. group ε consisted of five stutterers. group c comprised five nonstutterers. criteria for subject selection. 1. age: all ss were between twenty and twenty-six years of age to exclude the possibility of the changing 'stuttering mechanisms' of adolescence, and in view of the documented reliability of the ssw with 11-60 year olds.6 2. home language: the home language of all ss is south african english — to prevent the influence of unfamiliarity with the language affecting performance. 3. intelligence: ss with average intelligence were utilized — to eliminate contamination by the suggested relationship between intelligence and auditory abilities.5 all ss were studying at post-matriculation levels. 4. handedness: only right handed ss were utilized as most stutterers are reportedly right handed 4 and in view of the theory that a correlation between ear, brain and hand dominance exists.26 5. peripheral hearing status: all ss were required to have pure tone thresholds within normal limits and no otological involvement at the time of testing. 6. sex: each group consisted of one female and four males, as the reported ratio of male to female stutterers at the american 'college' level has been found to be 1,6 3 , 4 : 1 . 3 0 7. neurological status: as the presence of gross-neurological lesions would have biased results, their absence in all ss served as a further selection factor. t e s t s a n d p r o c e d u r e selection procedures. (a) speech task (for group e): in order to be included in this study nonfluencies as described by johnson et a l 1 2 had to be present in the speech of group ε on a propositional task. (b) handedness test: a handedness test based on that of satz et a l 2 6 was administered. subjects had to be right handed to be included in this study. (c) pure-tone air-conduction threshold test: thresholds were established in the conventional manner. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) central auditory testing in stutterers 103 audiological tests. a. pretests: speech reception threshold test (srt): written responses were required for this and all subsequent tests in view of the speech difficulty of group e. speech discrimination test: half lists of phonetically balanced words, the cid word list w-22 (pb cid w-22) were utilized in obtaining speech discrimination scores at 50 db sl for each ear. testing and scoring were carried out in the conventional manner.2 0 b. central auditory tests: the order of presentation of the central tests was randomized to prevent fatigue and learning from influencing results. (a) phonetically balanced cid w-22 with contralateral and ipsilateral masking at signal to noise ratios of 0 db and lodb. it has been reported that ipsilateral masking with pbs is a test sensitive to central disorders18 and the use of monaural tasks in brain stem testing has been suggested.11 the sub-test, including contralateral masking thus served as a basis of comparison. results of previous investigations indicate a greater breakdown in speech discrimination at a signal to noise (sn) ratio of greater than 5 db than at 0 db.3 7 as complexity of central tests has been stressed5 a sn ratio of— 10 db was incorporated, while the sub-test at the sn ratio of 0 db was utilized to serve as a basis of comparison. all sub-tests were administered at 50 db sl. white noise was utilized as it has been reported to be the most effective form of masking noise for speech.20 pb cid w-22 half lists were utilized to prevent fatigue on the part of s. conventional instructions and scoring procedures were employed.2·2 0 (b) the staggered spondaic word test. the ssw, reportedly a test of temporal lobe functioning,6 was administered at 50 db sl to each ear. an american recording of the ssw test list ec was utilized which consists of four practice items and 40 test items. each test item consists of two overlapping spondees, one presented to each ear, arranged so that the last syllable of the first spondee and the first syllable of the second spondee overlap in time. the non-competing syllables form a third spondee. presentation of the first syllable alternates between the ears and is preceded by 'are you ready?' preceding the test is a recorded 1 000 hz tone in each channel allowing for calibration. katz's 6 system of weighting was employed in scoring the tests, but all faulty responses, e y · ' if minimal, were considered as errors.1 equipment audiological assessments were carried out in an i.a.c. acoustic suite (series 1600) utilizing the maico model ma-24 dual channel audiometer. the speech material was routed to this audiometer from a viking 88 stereo tape recorder. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 104 gabrielle μ. karr results and discussion no significant differences in performance between group ε and group c were found on both central tests utilized. nevertheless performance of all ss taken as one group revealed several trends. discussion of the trends and observations made on the brainstem test will be followed by that of the temporal lobe test. (a)pb cid w-22 with ipsilateral and contralateral masking at sn ratios of 0 db and 1 0 db. the following trends were noted for the ss, taken as one group: 1. ipsilateral masking sub-tests yielded significantly fewer correct responses than contralateral masking (p< 0,001), validating the former's greater complexity. a similar finding is reported for performance on the swami, a test reportedly sensitive to brainstem lesions.35 2. significantly fewer correct responses occurred when the masking, whether ipsilateral or contralateral, was 10 db sl louder than the speech signal as opposed to speech and masking of equal sls (p< 0,001). this supports the conclusion that discrimination scores decrease as the sn ratio decreases.10 3. no ear differences occurred on the contralateral masking sub-tests while significantly fewer correct responses in the left ear occurred under the conditions of ipsilateral masking (0,025 < ρ . < 0,05) indicating that performance in the right ear was similar for both contralateral and ipsilateral masking sub-tests, while in the left ear, inferior performance under the ipsilateral masking condition occurred. performance on the contralateral masking condition was similar to that of the right ear. thus the process of transmission and figure-ground differentiation for speech appears to be more efficient when stimuli are presented to the right ear, which connects, by way of its strong contralateral pathway, to the left hemisphere which is dominant for speech.15 the strong contralateral pathway from the left ear leads to the right hemisphere which is dominant for nonspeech sounds.21 masking may be considered as one type of non-speech sound. speech and noise may be differentiated at the brainstem, whereafter the speech is transmitted to the left (speech) hemisphere, but if the brainstem does not function in this process, differentiation may only occur at the cortex where the rate of spontaneous activity and^the quantity of discharged impulses are less than at the brain s t e m , ' 1 0 , 2 3 thus placing the speech message transmitted from the left ear at a disadvantage. if this latter suggestion is to be adopted however, the validity of this test as a brainstem test is to be questioned. other limitations of this test may serve as further factors querying its validity as a brainstem test. for example, the necessity for central tests to be complex tasks has been discussed. the mere addition of masking noise may not have increased the complexity of this task sufficiently for it to assess brainstem functioning. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) central auditory testing in stutterers 105 (b) the staggered spondaic word test. the following trends were noted for the ss taken as one group. 1. significantly more errors occurred on competing than non-competing syllables (0,025 < ρ <0,01) confirming the greater complexity of dichotically than monotically presented material. 2. while performance on non-competing syllables was similar for both ears, the left ear showed significantly greater difficulty for competing stimuli than the right ear (p = 0,05). under competing conditions the rea discussed earlier may thus be considered to have occurred and, as no differences between group ε and group c were evident, no difference in the dominance between the stutterers and non-stutterers in this experiment was seen to exist — casting doubt on the applicability of the orton-travis theory to these stutterers. it has nevertheless been claimed that the ssw is 'free' from laterality effects.6if this holds true, the true dichotic properties of the ssw are to be questioned. the present study does however indicate the presence of the rea in the ssw. the true dichotic properties of the ssw are to be further questioned however. familiarity and word probability introduce the variable of redundancy, and, in central testing the necessity of reduced redundancy has been stressed.5 although katz 1 3 felt the presence of this property to be advantageous to the test the reality of this contamination may have caused responses such as the following: stimulus: washtub response: washtime stimulus: batboy response: badboy the writer suggests that in the absence of the non-competing syllables, the ssw might be a 'truer' dichotic task. a further factor to consider is that in a dichotic task, when stimuli contrast by one phoneme, the rea occurs,21 but the competing syllables in katz's ssw differ in a multiplicity of phonemes. further, if a dichotic pair share no features, performance of normals and left-and right brain damaged subjects is similar.21 thus the importance of phonetic control in dichotic testing is highlighted. 3. three out of five subjects of both group ε and group c presented with scores inferiorto the average score (0,8) reported for an american group of comparable age on list ib of the ssw a predecessor of list ec, 1 4 with three ss in group ε and one s in group c falling more than one standard deviation from this average. this highlights the necessity of a south african standardization of the ssw. 4. error trends on the ssw were considered for all the ss as a group. it would have been of value to compare error trends between the groups in view of the finding that dominance for vowels and consonants differs between stutterers and non-stutterers.32 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 0 6 gabrielle μ. ka trends observed included: (a) reversals: (this is not considered an error 1 4 ). if the stimulus sequence is right non competing (rnc), right competing (rc) and left competing (lc), left non competing (lnc), the response sequence in a reversal is lc-lnc-rnc-rc. if one word leads another by 30-90 msecs, the latter word is perceived more clearly — this is known as the 'lag effect'16 and may have intervened in reversal responses. (b) omissions of both or either competing syllable: faulty attention, storage or output have been postulated as the cause hereof.21 the writer contends that the additional processes of perception and integration may intervene. (c) portmanteau terms: the competing stimuli were combined to form a single word, e.g. stimulus: (rc) white (lc) foot response: fight it has been suggested that on dichotic tests, errors produced by the dominant ear are the result of the interaction between the dominant and non-dominant ears.28 it is important to remember that in the ssw an re a for competing stimuli occurred. (d) substitutions, additions and omissions: competing condition: (i) vowel substitutions resulting in a non-meaningful spondee. (ii) additions, omissions or substitutions of a consonantal phoneme resulting in a meaningful spondee. (iii) additions, omissions or substitutions of a consonantal phoneme resulting in a nonmeaningful spondee. (iv) substitution of a syllable (possibly more familiar) resulting in a meaningful spondee. non-competing condition: fewer errors occurred on non-competing conditions nevertheless substitutions; omissions and additions of consonants and vowels were noted. from the above observations, it is evident that an in-depth analysis of errors on the ssw would entail not only phonemic but semantic considerations. the intervention of both these systems contaminates the dichotic task, as responses are not merely based on integration and storage abilities. the use of dichotic nonsense syllables may have eliminated this contamination16 although this, too, could involve unaccountable variables.13 the proposed central auditory test could utilize in dichotic presentation, stops (which are better identified, normally, in the right ear), / a / which shows right ear superiority and i i / which does n o t . 1 6 the utilization of right dominant sounds is hypothesized to be sensitive to left temporal lobe functioning. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) central auditory testing in stutterers 107 the ssw does incorporate a temporal element and includes the aspect of storage, which is important in central testing. 2 6 ' 2 8 nevertheless, it has been suggested that sentence use assesses the temporal nature of speech to a greater extent than word use. 2 9 it is important to bear in mind that temporal sequence perception is a function of the dominant hemisphere.3 in a natural sentence, however, two or three 'key words' may convey the sentence's meaning. to overcome this problem in auditory testing, the synthetic sentence identification test (ssi) was devised.29 in utilizing these sentence types in central auditory testing, a competing speech message was introduced, and it was concluded that the ssi presented with a contralateral competing message (ssi-ccm) is sensitive to temporal lobe lesions, while the ssi presented with an ipsilateral competing message (ssi-icm) is sensitive to brainstem lesions.11 familiar words were used however and the writer suggests that some probability factors may thus intervene in their perception. it is thus felt that a test containing strings of nonsense words presented dichotically should be considered, as the use of dichotic sentences with limited redundancy and controlled semantic value in central testing has been stressed.3 further factors which could account for the similar performance of the stutterers and nonstutterers in this study concern the 'nature' of stuttering, and include: 1. the possible existence of sub-populations of stutterers.2s' 3 3 2. the possibility of cerebral involvement other than temporal lobe or brainstem impairment, e.g. apraxia 8 , pyknolepsy34 or minimal cerebral dysfunction.24 3. the possibility of audiological involvement other than central impairment in stutterers, e.g. differing middle ear functioning7 or disturbed auditory monitoring.19 4. the possible aspects of validity of the emotional or behavioural theories of stuttering which have been postulated. conclusion the performance of stutterers and non-stutterers on pb cid w-22 auditory lists with ipsilateral and contralateral masking at signal to noise ratios of 0 db and 10 db and on the ssw were not significantly different. these results suggest the comparable functioning of the ss on the tests utilized. in accounting for these findings, the possibility of faults inherent in these tests, or of impairment in stutterers other than that of central auditory processes, must be considered. bearing in mind, however, the report of inferior performance of stutterers on the ssi-icm31 and the importance of the utilization of a test battery in audiological assessment,35 the inclusion of the ssi-icm for brain-stem lesions and the use of either the ssi-ccm or dichotic nonsense word sentences might have led to differential responses in the subjects. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 108 gabrielle μ. karr references 1. β alas, r. and simon, g. (1964): the articulation function of a staggered spondaic word list for anormal hearing population./. aud. res 4, 285-289. 2. berger, k. w. (1971): speech audiometry. in audiological assessment, rose, d. e. (ed). prentice-hall inc., n . j . 3. berlin, c. i. and lowe, s. s. (1975): temporal and dichotic factors in central auditory testing. in handbook of clinical audiology, katz, j. (ed). the williams and wilkins co., baltimore. 4. bloodstein, 0 . (1969): a handbook on stuttering. national easter seal society for crippled children and adults, chicago. 5. bocca, e. and calearo, c. (1963): central hearing processes. in modern developments in audiology, jerger, j. (ed). academic press inc., new york. 6. brunt, m. a. (1975): the staggered spondaic word (ssw) test. in handbook of clinical audiology, katz, j. (ed). the williams and wilkins co., baltimore. 7. butler, b.r. and stanley, p. e. (1966): the stuttering problem considered from an automatic control point of view. folia phoniat 18 33-44. 8. caplan, l. (1970): an investigation of some aspects of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic patients. unpublished research report, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. 9. dorman, m. f. and porter, r. j. jr. (1975): hemispheric lateralization for speech perception in stutterers. cortex, 11(2), 181-185. 10. findlay, r. c. and schuchman, g. i. (1976): masking level difference for speech: effects of ear dominance and age. audiology, 15(3), 232241. 11. jerger, j. (1973): diagnostic audiometry. in modern developments in audiology, jerger, j. (ed). academic press, new york. 12. johnson, w„ darley, f. l. and spriestersbach, d.c. (1963): diagnostic methods in speech pathology. harper and row, new york. 13. katz, j. (1962): the use of staggered spondaic words for assessing the integrity of the central auditory nervous system. /. aud. res 2 327-337. 14. katz, j. (1968): the sswtest: an interim report./. speech hear. dis 33(2), 132-146. 15. kimura, d. (1967): functional dominance of the brain in dichotic listening. cortex, 3(2), 163-178. 7 16. lowe, s. s„ cullen, j. k. jr., berlin, c. i., thompson, c. l. and willett, μ. e. (1970): perception of simultaneous dichotic and monotic monosyllables./. speech hear. res., 13(4), 812-822. 17. luria, a. r. (1973): the working brain. the penguin press, london. 18. milner, b. (1962): laterality effects in audition. in interhemispheric relations and cerebral dominance, mountcastle, v. b. (ed). the john hopkins press, baltimore. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) central auditory testing in stutterers 109 19. mysak, e. d. (1960): servo theory and stuttering. / . speech heardis., 25(2), 188-195. 20. newby, h. a. (1972): audiology. prentice-hall inc., new jersey. 21. oscar-berman, m., zurif, ε. b. and blumstein, s. (1975): effects of unilateral brain damage on the processing of speech sounds. brain and lang., 2(3), 345-355. 22. penfield, w. and roberts, l. (1959): speech and brain mechanisms. princeton university press, new jersey. 23. radionova, e. a. and vartanian, i. a. (1971): comparative description of the characteristics of normal activity at different levels of the auditory system./. aud. res., 11(3), 195-217. 24. saffer, d. (1976): personal communication. neurologist, baragwanath hospital, johannesburg. 25. sander, ε. k. (1975): untangling stuttering. a tour through the theory thicket. asha, 17(4), 256. 26. satz, p., achenbach, k„ pattishall, e. and tennell, e. (1965): order of report, ear asymmetry and handedness in dichotic listening. cortex, 1(4), 377-396. 27. slorach, n. and noehr, b. (1973): dichotic listening in stuttering and dyslalic children. cortex., 9(3), 295-300. 28. speaks, c„ gray, t., miller, j. and rubens, a.b. (1975): central auditory deficits and temporal lobe lesions. / speech hear. dis., 40(2), 192-205. 29. speaks, c. and jerger, j. (1965): method for measurement of speech identification./. speech hear. dis., 8(2), 185-194. 30. timmons, b. a. and boudreau, j. p. (1972): auditory feedback as a major factor in stuttering. / . speech hear. dis., 37(4), 476-484. 31. toscher, μ. m. and rupp, r. r. (1975): a study of the central auditory processes in stutterers using the synthetic sentence identification (ssi) test battery. asha, 17(9), 680. 32. tsunoda, t. and moriyana, h. (1972): specific pattern of cerebral dominance for various sounds in adult stutterers. / aud. res., 12(3), 216-227. 33. van riper, c. (1971): the nature of stuttering. prentice-hall inc., new jersey. 34. west, r. (1958): an agnostic's speculations upon stuttering. in stuttering: a symposium, eisenson, j. (ed). harper and row, new york. 35. willeford, j. a. (1969): audiological evaluation of central auditory disorders part ii. maico audiological library series, 6,5-7. 36. willis, c. (1975): stuttering and aphasia: some important similarities and some important differences. wmuj. ofsp. th„ 12(1), 11-12. 37. young, i.'m. and harbert, f. (1970): noise effects on speech discrimination score. / aud. res., 10(2), 127-131. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 29 die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur by verbale apraksie a van der merwe, d phil (pretoria) i c uys, d phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria j μ loots, d sc (fisiologie) (pretoria) instituut vir sportnavorsing, universiteit van pretoria r j grimbeek, β sc (hons) (pretoria) departement statistiek, universiteit van pretoria l ρ c jansen, d sc (fisika) (pretoria) privaat praktiserende akoestikus opsomming die konsep van konteks-sensitiwiteit soos ontleen aan die koalisiemodel hougroot rrwontlikhede in vir die interpretasie van verbaal apraktiese simptome en vir 'n beter begrip van die aard van die afwyking. in hierdie ondersoek wat deel uitmaak van die groter ondersoek waarin die effek van variasie in kontekstuele faktore op verbaal apraktiese spraak nagegaan word (van der merwe, uys, loots en grimbeek, 1987; 1988), word die effek van die kontekstuele faktore, klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur akoesties ontleed. vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en een persoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie is as proefpersonegebruik. daar is bevind dat stemaanvangstyd nie konteks-sensitiefis nie. die stemaanvangstydfoute is nie ware vervangings met stemlose klanke nie, maar is onkonstante distorsies weens oorskryding van die kritiese temporale ekwivalensiegrense vir interartikulator-sinchronisasie. vokaalduur en uitingduur, is wel konteks-sensitief. namate die moeilikheidsgraad van die uiting toeneem, neem die afwyking in duur ook toe. die temporale eienskappe van verbaal apraktiese spraak kan dus ook soos die ouditief waarneembare foute op grond van kontekssensitiwiteit verdeel word in kernsimptome en geassosieerde simptome. / / ' abstract the concept of context sensiti dtyas borrowed from the coalition model has important implications for the interpretation of the symptoms of apraxia of speech and for a better understanding of the nature of the disorder. in this study which was part of the wider investigation into the effect of variation in contextual factors on apraxia of speech (van der merwe, uys, loots and grimbeek, 1987; 1988), the φα of two contextual factors namely sound structure and articulatory features on voice onset time, vowel duration and utterance duration was acoustically analysed. four subjects with[acquired apraxia of speech and one with developmental apraxia of speech were tested. findings indicated that voice onset time is not context sensitive. errors in voice onset time were not true substitutions with voiceless sounds but were inconsistent distortions due to an inability to keep interarticulator synchronization within the critical temporal boundaries of motor equivalence. vowel and utterance duration were found to be context sensitive. the deviation in duration increased with increased complexity of the utterance. as was the case with the auditorily perceived symptoms, the temporal characteristics of apraxia of speech can also be classified into core symptoms and associated symptoms based on their context sensitivity. sistematiese variasie in die kontekstuele faktore, klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe van 'n spraakuiting het 'n invloed op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie en aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking word daardeur verkry (van der merwe, uys, loots & grimbeek, 1987 en 1988). die vraag bestaan of variasie in hierdie kontekstuele faktore 'n akoesties aantoonbare invloed sal he op die temporale eienskappe van verbaal apraktiese spraak en of dit ook aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking sal verskaf. dit is bekend dat temporale versteurings een van die mees kenmerkende eienskappe van verbale apraksie is (kent & rosenbek, 1983; collins, rosenbek & wertz, 1983; itoh&sasanuma, 1984) en variasie in die graad en tipe aantasting sal moontlik waargeneem word met 'n variasie in bogenoemde kontekstuele faktore. akoestiese ondersoeke van die temporale eienskappe van verbaal apraktiese spraak waarvan verslag gedoen is in die literatuur is algemeen gemik op simptoombeskrywing (wertz, la pointe & rosenbek, 1984). stadige spraakspoed, verlengde vokaalduur en swak koordinasie van stemgewing met ander artikulasiegebeure word as kenmerkende simptome van verbale apraksie beskryf (blumstein, cooper, caramazza & zurif, 1977; freeman, sands & harris, 1978; kent & rosenbek, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikadeafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 a va der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots, r j grimbeek en l ρ c jansen 1983). die konteks waarin hierdie eienskappe ondersoek is, is egter in geen van hierdie studies gekontroleer en gevarieer nie. die teoretiese belang van kontekstuele faktore is volledig bespreek in van der merwe et al. (1987). die konsep van kontekssensitiwiteit is ontleen aan die koalisiemodel wat groot moontlikhede inhou vir toepassing op verbale apraksie (kelso & tuller, 1981; kelso, tuller & harris, 1983). variasie in kontekstuele faktore stel differensiele eise aan die beplanning en uitvoering van gedrag en die effek wat hierdie variasie op die waargenome simptome het, kan insiggewende gegewens verskaf oor die aard van die afwyking. dit hou verder ook implikasies in vir die interpretasie van simptome en vir verdere navorsing. die resultate van die ondersoek waarin die invloed van kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie (van der merwe, et al. 1987, 1988) nagegaan is, is die simptome van verbale apraksie wel in 'n nuwe en insiggewende perspektief gestel. dit wil voorkom asof die simptome van verbale apraksie op grond van kontekssensitiwiteit verdeel kan word in kemsimptome en geassosieerde simptome. in die huidige ondersoek wat deel uitmaak van die groter ondersoek waarin die effek van variasie in kontekstuele faktore op verbaal apraktiese simptome nagevors is, word die effek van sistematiese variasie op sekere temporale aspekte van verbaal apraktiese spraak akoesties ondersoek. metode doel die doel van die ondersoek is om te bepaal of sekere kontekstuele faktore in spraakproduksie, naamlik die klankstruktuur (foneemstruktuur) en die artikulasie-eienskappe (motoriese kompleksiteit) van 'n uiting 'n effek het op die volgende temporale aspekte van die spraak van persone met verbale apraksie: interartikulator-sinchronisasie soos waargeneem in stemaanvangstyd. segmentele tydsduur soos waargeneem in vokaalduur. die spoed van spraak soos waargeneem in die duur van die uiting. eksperimentele ontwerp. 'n tweegroepontwerp waarin die invloed van gekontroleerde stimuli op die temporale eienskappe van verbaal apraktiese spraak bepaal is deur vergelyking met die invloed op die spraak van afgepaarde kontrolepersone, is gebruik. die kontrolepersone is afgepaar met betrekking tot ouderdom, geslag en spreektaal. die proefpersone, materiaal en prosedure van hierdie ondersoek is reeds volledig beskryf in 'n voorafgaande artikel (van der merwe et al. 1987) en word dus nie hier herhaal nie. kortliks kan wel gemeld word dat vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en een persoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie as proefpersone gebruik is. die kriterium is gestel dat 'n suiwer verbale apraksie vertoon moet word. wat die materiaal betref, is onsineenhede wat gekontroleer is in klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe ontwikkel (van der merwe, 1986). die vyf klankstruktuurgroepe (s) hetelkagt eenhede bevat wat in vier artikulasie-eienskapgroepe (a) verdeel is. vir die doel van die akoestiese analise is ses herhalings van vier eenhede in elke klankstruktuurgroep ontleed. apparaat vir die akoestiese analise. die akoestiese analise is uitgevoer in die forensiese akoestieklaboratorium van die suid-afrikaanse polisie. unieke apparaat wat ontwikkel is by die w.n.n.r. is vir die analise gebruik. hierdie apparaat wat skematies voorgestel word in figuur 1 is verkies bokant die spektrograaf (wat ook besktkbaar was) omdat dit die spraaksein op so 'n wyse kodeer op die ossiloskoop dat akkurate metings tot op die naaste millisekonde gemaak kan word. 'n sony tc 1552-bandopnemer voer die spraaksein na die digitale herhalingslyn model 2a. die beeld word vertoon op die skerm van 'n hewlettpackard 1200 b-tipe ossilloskoop. daar is na die spraaksein geluister met 'n w770-kopstuk. 1 oorfone figuur 1: skematiese voorstelling van die apparaat vir die akoestiese analise. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) invloed van sekere ontekstuele faktore op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur 3 1 prosedure vir die akoestiese analise. opleiding in die tegniek van akoestiese metings en die hantering van die akoestiese apparaat is ontvang van 'n akoestiese ingenieur, 'n kenner op die gebied van sprekerherkenning en akoestiese analises, wat dele van die apparaat ontwikkel het vir die w.n.n.r. tydens die analises is alle probleemgevalle met hierdie persoon bespreek. die akoestiese metings is nie op al die eenhede toegepas nie, maar slegs op eenhede 1 tot 4 van elke klankstruktuurgroep. die aantal eenhede in elke klankstruktuurgroep is vir die akoestiese ontledings beperk omdat dit duidelik geword het tydens ontleding dat die addisionele data geen verskil sal maak aan die uitslag van die ondersoek nie. die akoestiese ontledings is ook tydrowend. elk van die twintig uitings wat akoesties ontleed is, is ses keer herhaal en die meting van hierdie honderd en twintig uitings het ongeveer twintig uur per persoon geduur. die data van twee van die drie engelssprekende dames wat as kontroles opgetree het, is nie volledig ontleed nie. nadat die analise van die eerste dame voltooi is, is steekproefontledings op die data van die ander twee gedoen. daar is bevind dat die resultate grootliks ooreenkom en op aanbeveling van jansen (1985) is die data van die eerste dame as kontrole vir die drie engelssprekende proefpersone gebruik. slegs ouderdom is in hierdie geval nie konstant gehou nie. jansen (1985) beskou die data van een normale spreker as verteenwoordigend van normale spraak in die spesifieke taalgroep. die data van die ander twee kontrolepersone is volledig ontleed. die spraakseinopnames van die kontrolepersone is eerste ontleed ten einde die tegniek volkome te bemeester alvorens die meer atipiese data van die proefpersone ontleed is. voor elke meetsessie is vorige metings op 'n steekproefbasis herhaal ten einde die akkuraatheid te kontroleer. dit het ook 'n oefengeleentheid gebied voordat die verdere metings voortgesit is. die drie akoestiese metings van elke uiting is agtereenvolgens gedoen omdat dit die mees betroubare en tydbesparende metode is. i / / die drie metings is direk vanaf die ossilloskoopskerm in millisekondes gedoen. die spraaksein is na die ossilloskoop gevoer soos uiteengesit in figuur 1. die gedeelte van die spraaksein wat in die digitale herhalingslyn se geheue ingelees en daar , vasgevang is, is voortdurerid herhaal. hierdie digitale herhalingslyn stoor 2,5 sekondes spraak vir analise. die spoed waarteen die beeld oor die skerm beweeg, kan verstel word van vyf tot twintig (of meer) millisekondes per divisie (wat aangebring is op die skerm) afhangende van die instelling wat die duidelikste beeld weergee by 'n spesifieke uiting. die ondersoeker kan ook terselfdertyd na die herhaling luister. die tydsbasis van die ossilloskoop is gekalibreer en die metings kan direk van die skerm af gedoen word deur die aantal divisies op die skerm te tel. hierdie getal is dan met die ingestelde aantal millisekondes per divisie van die ι tydsbasis vermenigvuldig om die tydsduur in millisekondes te verkry. die meting van stemaanvangstyd. meting van die stemaanvangstyd van die inisiele konsonante / b / en / d / is gedoen. die stemaanvangstyd word gemeet vanaf die begin van die ploffing tot by die aanvang van stemgewing vir die vokaal. dit word as 'n positiewe waarde genoteer. soms is daar 'n stemvoorloop en die meting word dan gedoen vanaf die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 36, 1989 die begin daarvan tot by die ploffing. in did geval word dit as 'n negatiewe waarde genoteer. tydens die meting van die eerste proefpersoon se data is bevind dat die norme van normale sprekers nie altyd voldoende riglyne verskaf vir eenvormige metings nie. daar is toe begin om 'n reeks sketse van atipiese voorbeelde saam te stel namate dit opgeduik het. by elke volgende probleemgeval is dan terugverwys na die reeks vir riglyne vir meting of die nu we voorbeeld is by die reeks ingevoeg. die ossilloskoopbeeld waarop die stemaanvangstyd gemeet is, word ge'illustreer in figure 2 tot 5. die metingspunte word ook aangedui. sat: -80 millisekondes (spoed: 20 millisekondes per divisie duur van beeld: 200 millisekondes) figuur 2: voorbeeld van 'n negatiewe stemaanvangstyd by 'n normale spreker. sat: + 10 millisekondes (spoed: 20 millisekondes per divisie duur van beeld: 200 milliesekondes) figuur 3: voorbeeld van 'n positiewe stemaanvangstyd by 'n normale spreker. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 a vail der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots, r j grimbeek e n l ρ c jansen sat: -176 millisekondes (spoed: 50 millisekondes per divisie duur van beeld: 500 millisekondes) figuur 4: voorbeeld van 'n negatiewe stemaanvangstyd w a t byna buite die normale perke val by 'n verbaal apraktiese spreker. sat: +40 millisekondes (spoed: 20 millisekondes per divisie duur van beeld: 200 millisekondes) figuur 5: voorbeeld van 'n positiewe, maar atipiese stemaanvangstyd by 'n verbaal apraktiese spreker. die meting van vokaalduur. die duur van die eerste vokaal / a / of h i is vanaf die ossilloskoopskerm in millisekondes gemeet. die meting van vokaalduur is oor die algemeen 'n betroubare prosedure wat minder probleemgevalle oplewer as stemaanvangstyd. die vokaal word aangedui deur 'n groot reelmatige kurwe met 'n lae frekwensie. die meting word vanaf die begin van die kurwe tot by die laagste punt waar die kurwe afplat, gedoen. die metingspunte van die vokaalduur word ge'fllustreer in figuur 6. vokaalduur: 164 millisekondes (spoed: 25 millisekondes per divisie duur van beeld: 250 millisekondes) figuur 6: voorbeeld van die vokaalduur van 'n normale spreker. die meting van die duur van die uiting. die duur van die kvkv en kvk (in die geval van klankstruktuur 4) gedeelte van die eenhede is gemeet. die meting van klankstruktuur 3 en 5 is beperk tot hierdie dele, omdat die roudata sodoende vergelykbaar is met die duur van klankstruktuur 1 en 2. die produksie van die langer eenhede deur die verbaal apraktiese sprekers het ook meestal die geheue van die digitale herhalingslyn, naamlik 2,5 sekondes, oorskry en dus meting van langer dele verhoed. die eerste metingspunt is die aanvang van die ploffing en die tweede punt is die laagste vlak wat bereik word by die afplatting van die kurwe van die tweede vokaal. die kvk duur is ook gemeet vanaf die aanvang van die inisiele ploffer tot by die einde van die ploffing van die finale konsonant. uitingduur: 920 millisekondes (spoed: 120 millisekondes per divisie duur van beeld: 1,2 sekondes) figuur 7: voorbeeld van die uitingduur van 'n verbaal apraktiese spreker. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur 33 dataverwerking die stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur van ses herhalings van die twintig eenhede wat akoesties ontleed is, is vir elke proefpersoon en kontrolepersoon verkry. alvorens die invloed van die twee kontekstuele faktore bepaal kon tvord, is 'n gemiddelde foutwaarde vir elke persoon bereken. die metode van berekening word vervolgens beskryf. bepaling van die gemiddelde foutwaarde vir stemaanvangstyd. die stemaanvangstydwaardes van -180 millisekondes tot +15 millisekondes is aanvaar as die normale perke. in die literatuur word 'n positiewe telling van +25 millisekondes as die hoogste normale perk beskou. daar is egter waargeneem dat by 'n telling hoer as +15 millisekondes die klank reeds stemloos klink. jansen (1985) beskou ook 'n telling hoer as +15 millisekondes as afwykend en daar is op hierdie waarde as die hoogste normale perk besluit. die data van die kontrolepersone is dus nie vir hierdie spesifieke doel gebruik nie. verskeie metodes is oorweeg om 'n enkele stemaanvangstydwaarde wat die foutgrootte sal aandui oor ses herhalings, te bereken. daar is volstaan deur 'n gemiddelde foutwaarde vir die ses herhalings van elke eenheid te bereken. dit is gedoen deur die hoeveelheid millisekondes bokant 15 of onder 180 (laasgenoemde het nooit voorgekom nie) oor die ses herhalings bymekaar te tel en deur ses te deel. hierdie gemiddeldes vir elke eenheid en vir elke proefpersoon word aangegee in tabel 1. die klankstruktuurgroepe (si tot s5) en die artikulasieeienskapgroepe (al tot a4) word in die tabel onderskei soos wat dit aangegee vyord in tabelle 2 en 3. " bepaling van die gemiddelde foutwaarde vir vokaalduur en uitingduur. die gemiddelde foutwaarde (die mate van afwyking vanaf die normale) is op dieselfde wyse bepaal vir vokaalduur en uitingduur en dit word dus gesamentlik bespreek. i die gemiddelde duur in millisekondes van die ses herhalings / v a n elke eenheid by elke proefpersoon en kontrolepersoon is bereken. die verskil tussen hierdie twee waardes wat die mate van afwyking vanaf die j normale weergee, is bereken. die waardes van die proefpersone was in alle gevalle groter as di6 die suid-afnkaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 van die kontrolepersone behalwe by enkele kvk-uitings. hierdie verskille in millisekondes word vir vokaalduur weergegee in tabel 4 en vir uitingsduur in tabel 5. die klankstruktuurgroepe en artikulasie-eienskapgrciepe word ook in hierdie geval onderskei op die tabelle. statistiese prosedure om te bepaal of klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe enige effek het op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur. ten einde te bepaal of klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe enige effek het of sat, vokaalduur en uitingduur, is die verdelingsvrye variansie-analiseprosedure van friedman gebruik. hierdie analise is toegepas op die waardes in die kolomme en rye van tabelle 1,4 en 5. die uiteensetting van die rye en kolomme van die tabelle is volgens die riglyne wat by die ontwikkeling van die materiaal bepaal is. deur middel van die friedman-prosedure is vasgestel of die gemiddelde foutwaardes van die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe (si tot s5) of die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe (al tot a4) betekenisvol van mekaar verskil. dit is globaal vir klankstruktuur en vir artikulasie-eienskappe bepaal en dit is ook vir die afsonderlike aen sgroepe bepaal. dit is essensieel om die invloed van klankstruktuur by elke artikulasiegroep afsonderlik te bepaal (dus vier vertikale vergelykings van die kolomwaardes in tabelle 1, 4 en 5) aangesien die artikulasieeienskappe van die uitings andersins ook 'n invloed op die waarde wat verkry word, sal uitoefen. dit geld ook vir die bepaling van die effek van artikulasie-eienskapgroepe (in hierdie geval is vyf horisontale vergelykings van die rywaardes gedoen). die invloed van die artikulasieeienskappe word dus by elke klankstruktuurgroep afsonderlik bepaal. die p-waardes toon die betekenisvolheid van verskille aan. resultate en bespreking resultate van stemaanvangstydmetings algemene beskrywing en bespreking. die meting van die stemaanvangstyd (sat) van ses herhalings van twintig eenhede deur die verbaal apraktiese sprekers wat in hierdie ondersoek gebruik is, dui daarop dat afwykende tabel 1: gemiddelde en totale stemaanvangstydfoutwaardee i n millisekondes klankstruktuur1 artikulasie-eienskapgroepe groepe • ' al a2 a3 a4 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 si eenhede: 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 si 0 0 4 0 0 0 0 0 0 4 3 0 12 0 1 2 0 0 0 0 26 s2 eenhede: 2.1 2.2 2.3 2.4 s2 1 0 1 0 1 3 0 2 0 1 1 0 24 0 1 9 0 9 0 1 54 s3 eenhede: 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 s3 10 2 5 0 0 0,5 0 0 0 13 2 0 5 0 1 4 0 2.0 0 0 26,5 eenhede: 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 5 1 0 0 3 1 0 0 0 0 5 0 11 4 0 4 0 11. "0 0. 45 s5 1 eenhede: 5.1 5.2 5.3 ,5.4 s5 1 5 19 6 0 2 0 0 0 0,5 0 2 0 0 1 0 7 0 0 42,5 totaal 63 26,5 27,5 68 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 a va der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots, r j grimbeek en l ρ c jansen stemaanvangstye (sate) voorkom by die meeste van hierdie sprekers. (kyk tabel 1 vir die gemiddelde sat-foute in millisekondes vir elke eenheid en vir elke proefpersoon en tabelle 2 en 3 vir voorbeelde van gemete waardes.) die resultate van hierdie studie stem in groot mate ooreen met die resultate van vorige ondersoeke na sat by verbaal apraktiese sprekers en addisionele waarnemings is ook gemaak (freeman et al. 1978; blumstein et al. 1977; hoit-dalgaard, murray & kopp, 1983; itoh en sasanuma, 1984; kent en rosenbek, 1983). tabel 2: die gemete sat-waardes van die ses herhalings van eenhede in die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe in artikulasie-eienskapgroep 1 tabel 3: die gemete sat-waardes van die ses herhalings van eenhede in die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe in klankstruktuurgroep 1. freeman et al. (1978) wat 'n gevallestudie uitgevoer het, meld dat daar onder meer bepaal moet word of afwykende sat 'n algemene eienskap van verbale apraksie is en of die probleem idiosinkraties is. in die huidige ondersoek is bevind dat nie alle sprekers wat wel as verbaal aprakties geklassifiseer kan word, sat-foute maakof in dieselfde mate maak nie. proefpersoon 2 het slegs by artikulasiegroep 1 en in besonder by s5 sat-foute gemaak (kyk tabel 1). proefpersoon 4 het slegs by uiting 4. 3 'n gemiddelde sat-fout van vier. verder vertoon hy geen sat-foute nie. die ander drie proefpersone maak wel sat-foute. die feit dat nie alle verbaal apraktiese sprekers sat-foute maak nie, dui moontlik daarop dat interartikulatorsinchronisasie soos onder meer weerspieel in sat, beheer word deur 'n meganisme wat nie in alle verbaal apraktiese sprekers versteur is nie. afwykende sat is dikwels die residuele probleem van 'n verbaal apraktiese spreker (freeman et al. 1978). hierdie gegewens impliseer dat selektiewe aantasting van temporale interartikulator-sinchronisasie kan voorkom en dui dus daarop dat hierdie komponent van beplanning moontlik onafhanklik funksioneer. hoit-dalgaard et al. (1983) noem dat satby verbale apraksie onvoorspelbaar is en ook kent en rosenbek (1983) meld dat die sat-resultate van die individuele sprekers verskil. hulle vind egter afwykings by al die sprekers. die feit dat nie alle sprekers in hierdie studie sat-foute vertoon nie, kan ook daarop dui dat verskillende tipes verbale apraksie voorkom, met selektiewe aantasting van komponente van motoriese beplanning. die sat-foute wat waargeneem is, is in alle gevalle te groot positiewe sat tellings van +15 millisekondes of meer vir stemhebbende klanke (kyk tabelle 2 en 3 vir voorbeelde). die positiewe telling is die gevolg van 'n verlengde stemnalooptyd. sat-tellings van meer as +15 millisekondes bring mee dat die klank as stemloos waargeneem word. die materiaal sluit slegs stemhebbende klanke in en afleidings kan dus slegs hieroor gemaak word. in die literatuur word gerapporteer dat stemhebbende klanke meer algemeen stemloos gemaak word as wat stemlose klanke stemhebbend gemaak word (wertz et al. 1984: 52). die waarneming dat stemhebbende klanke ίέ groot positiewe tellings oplewer van ongeveer +15 tot +30 millisekondes, bevestig die resultate van ander studies (freeman et al. 1978;. blumstein et al. 1977). daar is verder ook bevind dat slegs 17 van die 600 uitings (dus 2,8%) positiewe tellings van 35 millisekondes of meer getoon het. 'n stemlose, bilabiale plosief se sat is + 35 tot + 1 5 0 millisekondes (blumstein et al. 1977). 'n ware stemlose klank is dus slegs in 'n minimum van die gevalle geproduseer. freeman etal. (1978) het gevind dat d a a r ' n oor-! vleueling in die sate van stemhebbende en stemlose klanke voorkom en dat ook stemlose klanke dus nie volkome korrek geproduseer word nie. die ίέ groot positiewe tellings wat waargeneem is, impliseer op 'n gedragsvlak dat die glottale sluiting temporaal vertraag is in verhouding tot die verbreking van die afsluiting deur die artikulators. daar is verskillende moontlike verklarings vir die verlengde stemnalooptyd of anders gestel, die vertraagde inisiering van stemgewing wat voorkom by verbaal apraktiese sprekers. vanuit die oogpunt van die bekende regressieteorie wat ook soms op verbale apraksie toegepas word (marquardt, reinhart & peterson, 1979) kan daar gepoog word om hierdie verskynsel te verklaar as moontlike regressie na 'n infantiele vorm van spraakproduksie. volgens kewley-port en preston (1974) dek die sate van beide stemhebbende en stemlose klanke 'n wye omvang by kinders op 'n leeftyd van ses maande. daarna volg 'n periode waarin die sat vir stemhebbende en proefpersoon uitingnommer gemete sat-waardes van die ses herhalings 1 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 0. +11 0 0 0 0 + 10 0 0 +10 0 + 9 + 10 +20 +12 +20 +24 +10 + 10 +18 +10 +11 +12 +24 2 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 + 6 0 0 1 1 6 2 0 6 4 + 6 0 0 + 6 3 4 7 0 0 0 4 0 0 + 8 0 0 0 + 6 0 0 + 6 3 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 +26 +24 0 +20 +10 +16 0 0 +10 +12 +12 +12 + 18 +46 +20 +26 +22 +26 + 16 +14 +14 +10 +12 +10 4 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 6 0 6 0 4 0 3 6 2 0 8 0 8 0 4 8 8 0 8 0 0 8 0 1 0 0 5 2 2 2 7 6 4 8 0 6 4 6 0 4 8 1 0 0 8 0 5 2 5 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 + 5 +10 + 9 + 5 +12 +10 + 6 +20 +20 +20 +20 +18 + 6 +10 +14 + 7 +20 +12 + 6 + 1 4 + 8 + 6 0 + 1 0 proefpersoon uitingnommei gemete sat-waardes van die ses herhalings 1 1.1 2.1 3.1 4.1 5.1 0 +11 0 0 0 0 + 14 +10 +19 +14 +15 +18 + 18 +15 +20 +36 +20 +40 + 15 +20 +26 +15 +28 + 8 + 10 +11 +12 +13 +30 +32 2 1.1 2.1 3.1 4.1 5.1 + 6 0 0 1 1 6 2 0 6 4 0 0 2 6 0 +12 0 +26 + 6 + 6 0 + 6 0 +10 0 +10 +18 +16 +10 +15 +120 +10 +10 +20 +17 3 1.1 2.1 3.1 4.1 5.1 +26 +24 0 +20 +10' +16 0 0 +20 +10 +10 + 7 +12 +10 +14 +40 +20 +10 0 + 5 0 +16 + 5 +10 +24 +34 +10 +12 +16 +22 4 1.1 2.1 3.1 4.1 5.1 6 0 6 0 4 0 3 6 2 0 8 0 8 4 6 0 8 0 6 0 4 0 0 4 8 1 0 1 3 2 1 0 0 3 6 7 6 4 0 7 2 6 0 6 0 1 0 1 1 2 6 8 1 3 6 1 2 6 8 8 0 1 0 0 5 1.1 2.1 3.1 4.1 5.1 + 5 +10 + 9 + 5 +12 +10 + 5 + 9 +20 +10 +10 +10 + 10 +12 0 +12 0 +11 20 +10 0 0 + 7 + 7 vervang met /t/ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur 35 stemlose klanke net tussen 0 en +20 millisekondes is. stemvoorlope kom glad nie voor nie. volgens bogenoemde outeurs en cooper (1977) wat die teorie ondersteun, is dit motories maklik om 'n stemhebbende klank met 'n kort (0 +20 millisekondes) nalooptyd te produseer. die ploffing lei tot 'n afname in lugdruk wat die stembande dan in staat stel om spoedig na die ploffing te begin vibreer. 'n lang stemnalooptyd vir stemlose klanke vereis volgens hul verklaring 'n addisionele neurale bevel en is dus moeiliker om te produseer. oppervlakkig beskou, verklaar hierdie ontwikkelings-teorie nie die groot positiewe tellings (+15 tot +35 millisekondes) nie want daarvolgens neig die verbaal apraktiese sprekers om stemhebbende klanke motories moeiliker te maak. die belangrike punt wat hier in gedagte gehou moet word, is dat stemgewing soms in terme van die norm te laat 'n aanvang neem of ge'inisieer word vir die korrekte produksie van 'n stemhebbende klank. dit sou dus sinvol wees om aandag te skenk aan die neurale inisiering van stemgewing en gegewens in hierdie verband na te gaan vir moontlike verklarings. glottale bewegings geskied sonder die bewussyn van die normale spreker maar daar is wel neuro-anatomiese (jiirgens & ploog, 1981; ploog, 1981), neuropatologiese (jiirgens & von cramon, 1982) en neurofisiologiese (aitken & wilson, 1979) gegewens wat aandui dat fonasie willekeurig beheer word. die fonasiekontrolestelsel is hierargies georganiseerd en die hoogste vlak is gesetel in die anterior limbiese korteks (ploog, 1981). skynbaar speel ander strukture wat essensieel is vir artikulasie (byvoorbeeld die sensories-motoriese korteks, talairius en serebellum) nie 'n rol by stemgewing nie (jiirgens & ploog, 1981). die feit dat die limbiese stelsel wat 'n rol speel by inisiering van motoriek (mogeson, jones & yim, 1980) ook willekeurige stemgewing beheer, is in die lig van die feit dat verbale apraksie beskryf word as 'n afwyking in willekeurige beweging, belangrik. moontlik kan 'n versteuring in die meganisme van willekeurige inisiering deur die limbiese stelsel, of versteurde interaksie met ander motoriese dele, die afwyking in temporale sinchronisasie en wel die vertraagde inisiering van stemgewing verklaar. dit is insiggewend dat geen stemvoorlope waargeneem is by die drie proefpersone wat sat-foute gemaak het nie. hierdie oevinding bevestig die resultate van freeman et al. (1978). antisiperende stemgewing het dus glad nie voorgekom nie, maar slegs vertraagde stemgewing. ί vanuit 'n ander perspektief (wat nie teenstrydig is met die verklaring hierbo nie, maar dit wel aanvul) kan die temporale sinchronisering van artikulatoriese beweging met glottale sluiting, as 'n jnotoriese doelwit wat die spreker moet bereik, beskou word. die ossilloskoopbeeld wat verkry is (wat moontlik gemaak word deur die besondere apparaat wat gebruik is) by proefpersone 3 en 5 werp moontlik verdere lig op temporale sinchronisering as 'n motoriese doelwit. by beide hierdie persone was daar in uitings met hoe positiewe sat-tellings, aanduidings dat ploffing en stemgewing onafhanklik plaasvind en nie gei'ntegreera is nie. die plotting het in baie gevalle gepaard gegaan met baie ruising wat soms verleng was en dan 'n onafhanklike aanvang van stemgewing. dit wil voorkom asof daar 'η bewustheid by hierdie twee verbaal apraktiese sprekers is van die twee komponente van die uiting, naamlik ploffing en stemgewing. daar is dus aanduidings van kennis van die komponente van die motorprogram, maar daar ontstaan blykbaar 'n probleem in die vooruitvoering daarvan. die probleem kan die vertraagde inisiering van stemgewing wees of die onafhanklike vooruitvoering van subroetines van bewegings. die temporale sinchronisering van spraakbewegings het egter kritiese temporale ekwivalensiegrense en die onafhanklike of vertraagde inisiering van subroetines van bewegings bring mee dat temporale wanpassing plaasvind met gevolglike distorsiefoute. die kritiese grense van temporale interartikulatorsinchronisasie is in groot mate onveranderlik en dit stel 'n hoe eis aan die spreker om die sinchronisasie wat binne millisekondes moet plaasvind, te beheer in vinnige opeenvolging. so 'n uitgangspunt verklaar ook deels die onkonstantheid in produksie wat ook waargeneem is. 'n belangrike implikasie van die onkonstant foutiewe stemgewing is dat dit 'n baie sterk aanduiding is dat dit onwaarskynlik is dat die vervangings met stemlose klanke die gevolg is van foutiewe klankseleksie of foutiewe ouditiewe persepsie van stemhebbendheid. dit is aan die ander kant 'n baie sterk aanduiding dat stemhebbend-stemlose klankvervangings die gevolg is van afwykende beheer van die produksie van stemhebbende klanke. veranderlikheid of onkonstantheid in die verskillende komponente van 'n beweging is 'n kenmerk van motoriek en ook spraakmotoriek. die mate van veranderlikheid neem af tydens ontwikkeling totdat 'n volwasse stadium bereik word wanneer bewegings steeds veranderlik is, maar binne die grense van motoriese ekwivalensie (turvey, fitch & tuller, 1982; kelso et al. 1983: 145). die afleiding kan dus gemaak word dat onkonstantheid in die bewegingspesifikasies, byvoorbeeld in temporale inter-artikulator sinchronisasie, 'n aanduiding is van 'n motoriese probleem en nie 'n afwyking in die fonologiese kennis van fonotaktiese reels nie. kennis van die toepaslikheid van 'n spesifieke foneem en die ouditiewe herkenning daarvan is waarskynlik meer stabiele vermoens wat berus op verworwe reels en is nie wisselend van moment tot moment nie. afleidings gegrond op 'n enkele produksie van 'n klank is dus nie verteenwoordigend van die ware vermoe of onvermoe van die verbaal apraktiese spreker nie. 'n enkele produksie van 'n stemhebbende klank wat toevallig stemloos klink, kan die luisteraar mislei en meebring dat die afwyking gei'nterpreteer word as 'n klankvervanging en dus afwykende klankseleksie wat aanduidend kan wees van 'n fonologiese beplanningsprobleem. 'n interessante bevinding wat die uitgangspunt in hierdie studie bevestig, is dat die sate slegs in 2,8% van die uitings die waardes van ware stemlose klanke, naamlik +40 millisekonde en hoer (cooper, 1977) bereik het. die "ververvangings" met klanke wat stemloos klink, is dus nie ware vervangings nie maar wel distorsie van die klanke. 'n verdere insiggewende bevinding is dat die sat-resultate van die proefpersoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie volkome ooreenstem met die resultate van die ander proefpersone. proefpersoon 5 het ook onkonstant sat-foute gemaak en hy het die sat-kenmerke vertoon wat ooreenstem met die patroon van proefpersoon 3. dit wil dus voorkom asof dieselfde meganisme versteur word by kongenitale en verworwe breinskade wat aanleiding gee tot verbale apraksie. opsommend kom die sat-resultate daarop neer dat afwykings nie in dieselfde mate by alle proefpersone voorkom nie; dat die satfoute die vorm aanneem van hoe positiewe tellings van +15 tot +35 millisekondes; dat sat onkonstant foutief is en dat die resultate van die proefpersoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie ooreenstem met die resultate van die ander proefpersone. die sat-foute word onvoldoende verklaar deur die regressieteorie of'n teorie waarin die foute aan ware klankvervangings of persepsieprobleme toegeskryf word. die resultate word meer sinvol verklaar as 'n onvermoe om interartikulatorsinchronisasie binne die kritiese grense die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 a va der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots, r j grimbeek en l ρ c jansen van temporale ekwivalensie te beheer en die bewegings temporaal georden vinnig te inisieer en gei'ntegreerd vooruit te voer. die invloed van die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting op stemaanvangstyd. ten einde te bepaal of variasie in die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting 'n effek het op die stemaanvangstyd van inisiele stemhebbende konsonante by verbaal apraktiese sprekers, is 'n tweerigting-variansie-analise toegepas op die gemiddelde satfoute van eenhede uit die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe. die eenhede vanuit die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe is afsonderlik vergelyk. 'n p-waarde van kleiner as 0,0125 dui daarop dat daar betekenisvolle verskille (op die 5% peil van betekenis) tussen die gemiddelde foute van die eenhede voorgekom het. die p-waardes is almal groter as 0,0125 wat aandui dat daar geen betekenisvolle verskille tussen die gemiddelde satfoute in die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe is nie. die feit dat sat nie betekenisvolle sensitiwiteit vertoon vir die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting nie, kan 'n aanduiding wees dat die beheer van sat 'n suiwer motoriese gebeure is. die motoriese prosesse wat nodig is om sat te beheer, vertoon dus 'n afwyking ongeag die uiting waarin dit moet plaasvind. die onkonstantheid in die sat-foute het reeds gedui op hierdie „ moontlikheid. dit wil dus op grond van hierdie ontleding voorkom asof sat nie konteks-sensitief is nie en 'n suiwer motoriese komponent van die afwyking in verbale apraksie vorm. die afwesigheid van konteks-sensitiwiteit dui moontlik ook daarop dat afwykings in sat 'n kernsimptoom van verbale apraksie weerspieel. die invloed van die artikulasie-eienskappe van 'n uiting op stemaanvangstyd. ten einde te bepaal of variasie in die artikulasie-eienskappe van uitings met dieselfde klankstruktuur enige effek het op sat, is 'n tweerigting-variansie-analise uitgevoer op die gemiddelde satfoute van eenhede uit die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe. alle p-waardes is bokant 0,0125 wat impliseer dat daar geen betekenisvolle variasie tussen die uitings in die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe is nie. die afleiding gegrond op hierdie analise is dus dat sat nie sensitief is vir die verskillende artikulasie-eienskappe wat in die materiaal teenwoordig is nie. dit wil dus voorkom asof die afwyking in interartikulator-sinchronisasie vir sat by die verbaal apraktiese spreker voorkom ongeag die artikulasieof klankstruktuur-eienskappe van die uiting. foute kom egter nie tydens elke uiting voor nie. die verbaal apraktiese spreker het skynbaar 'n probleem om hierdie aspek van spraakbewegings te beheer. resultate van vokaalduurmetings. algemene beskrywing en bespreking. wisselende grootte verskille tussen die gemiddelde vokaalduur van ses herhalings van die verbaal apraktiese sprekers en normale sprekers is waargeneem. hierdie verskille in millisekondes word aangegee in tabel 4. dit is insiggewend dat daar by sommige eenhede geen verskille waargeneem is nie. die uitings van die verbaal apraktiese sprekers was selfs in enkele gevalle 'n paar millisekondes korter as άίέ van die normale sprekers en die verskil word dan as nul aangegee. tydens die verwerking van die data is ook waargeneem dat indien die langste duur van die ses herhalings van die normale sprekers in berekening gebring word, daar kleiner verskille veral by s4 voorkom. dit is ook interessant dat daar groot verskille bestaan tussen die mate waarin die individuele sprekers afwyk van die normale sprekers. proefpersoon 4 vertoon die grootste mate van afwyking. hy het ook by die sat-metings anders gereageer as die ander vier proefpersone en dit wil dus voorkom asof hierdie spreker wat tog as verbaal aprakties gediagnoseer kan word, moontlik 'n ander tipe aantasting vertoon. proefpersone 1 en 5 vertoon die kleinste mate van afwyking ten opsigte van vokaalduur. die proefpersoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie vertoon wel ook verlengde vokaalduur soos die ander proefpersone. dit is belangrik om daarop te wys dat alhoewel daar individuele verskille voorkom, die orde van die foute by die verskillende eenhede, ooreenstemming toon. die faktore wat in die materiaal gemanipuleer is, het dus 'n ooreenstemmende uitwerking op al die proefpersone. die foutgrootte by s4 is byvoorbeeld kleiner as by die ander strukture vir al die proefpersone. dit kom dus voor asof daar 'n gemeenskaplike probleem by die sprekers aanwesig is alhoewel individuele verskille voorkom. in die literatuur word oor die algemeen gerapporteer dat verlengde vokaalduur voorkom by die verbaal apraktiese spreker en dat dit 'n komponent is van algemene artikulasieverlenging wat kenmerkend is van hierdie sprekers (freeman et al. 1978; collins et al. 1983; kent & rosenbek 1983). die mate van afwyking word nie aangegee nie en die struktuur van die uitings is ook nie gekontroleer nie. kent en rosenbek (1983) meld wel dat die duur toeneem namate die lengte van die uiting toeneem. duffy en gawle (1984) het vokaalduur in kvkuitings ondersoek en het ook vokaalduur korter as die normale waargeneem. die moontlikheid bestaan dat die wisselende grootte foute wat in die huidige studie waargeneem is, verband hou met die variasie in die klankstruktuur en artikulasieeienskappe van die uiting. die resultate van die ondersoek in hierdie verband word vervolgens verskaf. • die invloed van die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting op ! vokaalduur. i ι ten einde te bepaal of die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting 'n effell het op vokaalduur, is 'n tweerigting-variansie-analise uitge-! voer op die data wat aangegee word in tabel 4. globaal gesien1 is daar uitgesproke betekenisvolle verskille tussen die mate' van afwyking in vokaalduur van die eenhede in die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe. 'n p-waarde van 0,0000 is verkry. op grond van die beskikbare data kan die gevolgtrekking gemaak word dat die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting 'n effek het op die vokaalduur. vokaalduur vertoon dus konteks-sensiti; witeit. die betekenisvolle verskille wat hier waargeneem is, is tussen die vokaalduur van klankstruktuur 4 en die ander klankstrukture. die verskille tussen die ander klankstrukture is statisties nie betekenisvol nie. verbaal apraktiese sprekers vertoon feitlik normale vokaalduur in kvk-uitingsaferiengde segmentele duur is dus nie 'n primgre eienskap van verbaal apraktiese spraak nie (duffy & gawle, 1984; rosenbek, kent & lai pointe, 1984: 16). die mate van afwyking in vokaalduur het the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 37 invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur tabel 4: die verskil in millisekondes tussen die gemiddelde vokaalduur van die proefpersoon en die gemiddelde vokaalduur van die kontrolepersoon klankartikulasie-eienskapgroepe struktuurgroepe al a2 a3 a4 proefpersone 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 totaal totaal per persoon si eenhede: 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 1. 180 2. 539 3. 386 2697 4. 1017 5. 575 si 105 271 0 92 168 6 50 137 177 80 19 39 77 388 168 50 179 172 360 159 1. 180 2. 539 3. 386 2697 4. 1017 5. 575 s2 eenhede: 2.1 2.2 2.3 2.4 1. 97 2. 366 3. 731 2805 4. 1210 5. 401 s2 4 68 221 408 122 0 25 113 273 147 27 175 205 260 82 66 98 192 269 50 1. 97 2. 366 3. 731 2805 4. 1210 5. 401 s3 eenhede: 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 1. 222 2. 557 3. 832 3342 4. 1269 5. 462 s3 162 274 209 259 120 53 137 187 300 99 7 117 182 308 137 0 29 254 402 106 1. 222 2. 557 3. 832 3342 4. 1269 5. 462 s4 eenhede: 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 1. 84 2. 0 3. 199 545 4. 189 5. 73 s4 0 0 2 100 0 19 0 72 0 70 25 0 23 32 0 40 0 102 57 3 1. 84 2. 0 3. 199 545 4. 189 5. 73 s5 eenhede: 5.1 5.2 5.3 5.4 1. 304 2. 789 3. 860 3359 4. 1165 5. 241 s5 120 287 143 242 75 53 223 241 265 80 215 242 262 118 51 64 234 396 48 1. 304 2. 789 3. 860 3359 4. 1165 5. 241 totaal per persoon 391 900 575 1101 485 131 435 750 1015 396 158 546 729 1250 505 207 370 954 1484 366 totaal 3452 2727 3188 3381 toeneem. hul afleiding is dat die verbaal apraktiese spreker sensitiwiteit vir hierdie vlak van "fonologiese enkodering" behou. die spreker beskik dus nog oor die nodige kennis van linguistiese reels. die verskynsel dat vokaalduur verkort voor 'n stemlose plosief is 'n aangeleerde eienskap wat nie voorkom in die spraak van driejariges nie (disimoni, 1974). die feit dat hierdie verskynsel waargeneem word in die spraak van verbaal apraktiese sprekers is 'n verdere aanduiding dat die probleem nie bloot 'n regressie na 'n infantiele spraakproduksiewyse is nie. die invloed van die artikulasie-eienskappe van 'n uiting op vokaalduur. geeneen van die berekende p-waardes is onder 0,0125 nie, wat impliseer dat daar geen betekenisvolle verskille tussen die mate van afwyking in vokaalduur van eenhede in die verskillende artikulasiegroepe is nie. groot waardes is oor die algemeen verkry. die artikulasie-eienskappe wat in die materiaal gedek is, het dus geen statisties beduidende effek op die vokaalduur nie. die gegewens in tabel 4 toon egter aan dat die mate van afwyking van al en a2 meer verskil as wat byvoorbeeld al en a4 verskil. al vertoon die grootste mate van afwyking in vokaalduur en a2 die kleinste. daar is dus aanduidings dat eenhede met verskillende artikulasie-eienskappe nie homogene eise stel aan die spraakproduksievermoe van die verbaal apraktiese sprekers nie. ten einde duplisering te beperk sal die implikasies van hierdie gegewens bespreek word tydens die bespreking van die invloed van artikulasie-eienskappe op uitingduur. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 36, 1989 toegeneem namate die lengte van die uiting toegeneem het (kyk tabel 4). dit kom dus voor asof die spreker segmentele duur verleng in langer eenhede wat hoer eise stel aan sy spraakproduksievermoe. dit word algemeen aanvaar dat laiiger uitings lei tot meer spraakfoute by die verbaal apraktiese spreker (rosenbek et· al. 1984:15; kent & rosenbek, 1983). die resultate van hierdie studie bevestig dus hierdie aanname. dit beantwoordj ook in groot mate die vraag of verlengde artikulasie 'n direkte gevolg is vanjlie neuromotoriese afwyking (kent, netsell & abbs, 1979). op grond van die huidige resultate skyn dit nie die geval te wees nie. verlengde segmentele duur is meerj waarskynlik 'n kompensatoriese strategie om die onderliggende neuromotoriese probleem te oorkom. die resultate impliseer verder ook dat die engelssprekende verbaal apraktiese sprekers (dit is nie bekend of die reel ook geld in afrikaans nie) sensitiwiteit behou vir die linguistiese reel dat vokaalduur verkort voor 'n plosief in die finale posisie van 'n woord "(duffy & gawle, 1984: 169). dit word weerspieel deur die feitlik normale vokaalduur wat waargeneem is by die kvk-eenhede wat almal plosiewe klanke in die finale posisie het. alhoewel dit meestal onsineenhede was, het dieselfde reel wat verwag word by betekenisvolle spraak by beide groepe (eksperimenteel en kontrole) sprekers in werking getree. die resultate van collins et al. (1983) wat die behoud van 'n reel vir duurverhoudlnge waargeneem het, word dus bevestig. collins et al. (1983) het bevind dat alhoewel vokaalduur verleng is, die duur wel soos by die normale spreker verkort word namate die lengte van die woord waarin dit voorkom, r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a van der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots, r j grimbeek e n l ρ c jansen tabel 5: die verskil in millisekondes tussen die gemiddelde uitingduur van die proefpersoon en die gemiddelde uitingduur van die kontrolepersoon klankstruktuurgroepe artikulasie-eienskapgroepe klankstruktuurgroepe al a2 a3 a.4 proefpersone 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 5 totaal totaal per persoon si eenhede: 1.1 1.2 1.3 1.4 1. 1750 2. 2290 3. 2230 8199 4. 1370 5. 559 si 700 613 407 267 166 234 464 497 57 30 .486 613 740 326 130 330 600 586 720 233 1. 1750 2. 2290 3. 2230 8199 4. 1370 5. 559 s2 eenhede: 2.1 2.2 2.3 2.4 1. 710 2. 2078 3. 2070 7296 4. 2225 5. 213 s2 80 290 490 716 60 10 574 547 603 0 230 464 390 446 70 390 750 643 460 83 1. 710 2. 2078 3. 2070 7296 4. 2225 5. 213 s3 eenhede: 3.1 3.2 3.3 3.4 1. 2183 2. 3256 3. 3657 12927 4. 2997 5. 834 s3 953 940 1440 647 207 564 1090 897 670 197 433 663 717 720 233 233 563 603 960 197 1. 2183 2. 3256 3. 3657 12927 4. 2997 5. 834 s4 eenhede: 4.1 4.2 4.3 4.4 1. 719 2. 827 3. 409 2400 4. 330 5. "115 s4 140 417 30 210 0 87 107 74 0 37 136 176 76 80 53 356 127 229 40 25 1. 719 2. 827 3. 409 2400 4. 330 5. "115 s5 eenhede: 5.1 5.2 5.3 5.4 1. 2084 2. 3584 3. 3540 12662 4. 2777 5. 677 s5 750 1404 893 427 307 527 993 1267 747 363 793 733 610 226 444 394 647 993 144 1. 2084 2. 3584 3. 3540 12662 4. 2777 5. 677 totaal per persoon 2623 3664 3260 2267 740 1442 3228 3282 2077 264 1648 2709 2656 2182 712 1753 2434 2708 3173 682 totaal 12554 10273 9907 10750 resultate van uitingduurmetings. algemene beskrywing en bespreking. die verskil in millisekondes tussen die gemiddelde uitingduur van die proefpersone en die kontrolepersone word weergegee in tabel 5. die groot verskille wat waargeneem is, impliseer dat al die proefpersone stadige spraakspoed vertoon. volgens die meeste navorsers is stadige spraakspoed kenmerkend van die verbaal apraktiese spreker (wertz et al. 1984: 71; kent & rosenbek, 1983; rosenbek et al. 1984: 15). die grootste verskille tussen die gemiddelde uitingduur van normale en verbaal apraktiese sprekers ten opsigte van klankstruktuurgroepe kom voor by s3 en s5. daarna volg si en s2 en die kleinste verskil is by s4. wat die verskillende artikulasieeienskapgroepe betref, kom daar ook verskille voor wat groter is as die verskille wat ten opsigte van artikulasie-eienskapgroepe by die vokaalduurmetings waargeneem is (kyk tabel 4).-· wat die individuele proefpersone aanbetref, word bemerk dat proefpersoon 4 nie soos in die geval van vokaalduur die grootste afwyking vertoon nie. dit is 'n interessante verskynsel, want dit impliseer dat segmentele duur meer verleng is as by die ander sprekers, maar die oorgange van een klank na 'n ander vind dus vinniger plaas. 'n verdere interessante waarneming is dat proefpersoon 5 sonder uitsondering die kortste uitingduur van al die proefpersone yertoon. dit was egter nie die geval by vokaalduur nie (kyk tabel 4) alhoewel daar ook 'n neiging tot 'n vokaalduur korter as die gemiddelde waargeneem is. die oorgange tussen klanke vind dus by proefpersoon 5 vinniger plaas as by enige van die ander proefpersone. proefpersoon 5 wat 'n verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie vertoon, was nie-verbaal tot op onger veer vierjarige ouderdom en het daarna met behulp van die terapieprogram vir verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie (van der merwe, 1985) spraakproduksie aangeleer oor 'n tydperk van ongeveer vier jaar. 'n moontlike verklaring vir die vinniger spraakspoed is dat alle spraak wat hy vertoon doelbewus aangeleer is. een van die grondbeginsels van die terapieprogram is die doelbewuste herhaalde produksie van eenhede met toenemende spoed. die proefpersoon het dus 'n bewustelik aangeleerde motorprogram vir alle klanke en is geleer om dit herhaaldelik willekeurig te herroeop in alle klankomgewings en in langer wordende uitings. 'n mate van outomatisasie het waarskynlik plaasgevind. die ander proefper-, sone moet moontlik in 'n groter mate spraak wat voorheen outomaties plaasgevind het, doelbewus herroep, inisieer en beheer. dit kan die gevolg wees van die effek van verworwe breinskade nadat spraak reeds aangeleer is en' die reaksie daarop. dit kan andersyds ook toegeskryf word aan die feit dat die vier volwassenes vir korter periodes dieselfde tipe behandeling as proefpersoon 5 ontvang het en tydens die ondersoek the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur 39 nog stadige spraak en stadige inisiering vertoon het. die gebrekkige outomatisasie van spraak verklaar moontlik die stadiger spraakspoed wat veral in die oorgange tussen klanke voorkom. die afleiding kan dus nie sonder meer gemaak word dat kinders met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie vinniger spraakspoed vertoon (indien hulle wel spraak kan produseer) as gevalle met verworwe verbale apraksie nie omdat die vermoens waaroor proefpersoon 5 beskik deur middel van hierdie spesifieke benadering aan hom geleer is. dit is belangrik om daarop te wys dat proefpersoon 5 se spraak steeds stadiger is as normaal en dat die konteks van die uiting dieselfde uitwerking op sy spraakspoed het as op die spraakspoed van die persone met verworwe verbale apraksie. die mate van afwyking is die grootste by s3 en s5. daarna volg si en s2 wat in mindere mate 'n afwyking vertoon. die geringste uitval is by s4. metbetrekking tot spraakspoed vertoon die verbaal apraktiese spreker wel oppervlakkig geoordeel regressie. daar is eksperimenteel bevind dat woordduur by die jong kind langer is as by die ouer kind en die volwassene (kubaska & keating, 1981). die onderliggende redes hiervoor kan egter verskil. motoriese vaardigheid in die manipulasie van die spraakstrukture speel onder meer 'n rol by spraakspoed en die jong kind kan moontlik weens beperkte vaardigheid spraak stadiger produseer. stadige spraakspoed is ook kenmerkend van disartrie en in hierdie geval word spoed beperk weens versteurde spiertonus en onwillekeurige bewegings. 'n verlaging in spraakspoed kan ook kompensatories wees of weens gebrekkige outomatisasie. daar is dus verskillende redes vir stadige spraakspoed en dit is 'n oppervlakkige afleiding indien aanvaar word dat die stadige spraakspoed 'n simptoom van regressie na 'n infantiele spraakproduksiewyse is. opsommend kom die; resultate daarop neer dat die verbaal apraktiese sprekers wat in hierdie studie ondersoek is, stadige spraakspoed vertoon wat wissel in die mate waarin dit afwyk van normale spraakspoed. die rol van variasie in die konteks van die uiting by die wisselende mate van afwyking, word vervolgens bespreek. ι j die invloed van die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting op uitingduur die berekende p-waardes toon aan dat daar beduidende verskille voorkom tussen die mate van afwyking in uitingduur van eenhede in die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe. slegs by artikulasiegroep 4 het daar nie 'n beduidende verskil voorgekom tussen die klankstruktuurgroepe nie. die verskille wat waargeneem is, was tussen die eenhede in s4 en die in s3 en s5. betekenisvolle verskille is nie uitgewys tussen s4 en si en s2 nie, maar die numeriese totaalwaardes in tabel 5 toon tog aan dat si en s2 nie dieselfde invloed het op uitingduur as die ander strukture nie. dieselfde groepverdeling is waargeneem by vokaalduur (kyk tabel 4). die gevolgtrekking is dus dat die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting 'n invloed het op die mate van afwyking in uitingduur. 'n interessante verskynsel wat deur hierdie resultate aan die lig kom, is dat die byvoeging van 'n enkele klank by die klankstruktuur, meebring dat die produksie van die uiting meer problematies word vir die verbaal apraktiese sprekers. klankstruktuur 4 wat die minste foute tot gevolg gehad het (by alle akoestiese analises) het 'n kvk-struktuur terwyl klankstrukture 1 en 2 wat die tweede groep vorm 'n kvkv-struktuur het. daarteenoor het klankstrukture 3 en 5 wat die derde groep vorm onderskeidelik 'n kvkvken kvkvkvk-struktuur. die effek wat die byvoeging van 'n enkele klank het, is verrassend omdat die kvkv-struktuur byvoorbeeld slegs die byvoeging van 'n vokaal meebring. artikulatories gesproke vereis dit weinig addisionele artikulasiebewegings na die verbreking yan die afsluiting vir die finale plosief. fonologies gesproke is dit 'n klankstruktuur wat baie vroeg reeds in die spraak van die jong kind voorkom en die fonologiese beplanning van so 'n uiting behoort nie meer problematies te wees as die kvk-struktuur nie. 'n verklaring vir hierdie verskynsel word dus nie vanuit hierdie oogpunte verkry nie. 'n ander moontlike verklaring is dat die beplanning van 'n langer uiting meer kompleks is en meer tyd in beslag neem. klapp, anderson en berrian (1973) het'deur middel van 'n reaksietydstudie bevind dat dit langer neem om 'n tweelettergrepige woord te beplan as wat dit neem om 'n eenlettergrepige woord te beplan. die afleiding is dat dit langer neem om 'n meer komplekse motorprogram te beplan. die data in die huidige studie toon aan dat langer eenhede langer neem om te produseer. die feit dat korter eenhede teen feitlik normale spoed geproduseer word impliseer dus dat daar nie 'n primere motoriese probleem is wat lei tot stadige spraakspoed nie. dit is dus die beplanning van die langer uiting wat soveel meer tyd in beslag neem en nie die produksie daarvan nie. die moontlikheid bestaan ook dat die langer uiting op 'n ander wyse beheer word as die korter uiting wat minder kompleks is. die verbaal apraktiese spreker kan moontlik vanwee die verhoogde eise van 'n langer uiting, dit nie volledig beplan voor produksie nie en moet dan voortdurend tydens produksie beplan. schmidt (1982: 207) meld onder meer dat motories vaardige persone lang reekse bewegings vooraf kan beplan. die teenoorgestelde is dus moontlik, naamlik dat beperkte motoriese vaardigheid meebring dat net korter reekse vooraf beplan kan word. die beplanning van 'n langer eenheid is waarskynlik om verskeie redes meer kompleks. die byvoeging van addisionele klanke bring mee dat 'n groter getal kernmotorprogramme herroep moet word, die vereistes van ruimtelike en temporale adaptasie en koartikulasie neem toe en die groter getal bewegingspesifikasies moet in die korrekte volgorde vooruitgevoer word. meer opeenvolgende variasie in bewegings kom ook voor in langer uitings. dit is in die praktyk waargeneem dat k1v1k1v] -eenhede by verbaal apraktiese sprekers soos by die normaal ontwikkelende kind vroeer voorkom en meer korrek geproduseer word. die eenhede wat in hierdie studie gebruik is, is ki v1k1v2 en kivik2v2-eenhede wat toenemend meer variasie in die bewegingspesifikasies meebring. indien die mate van variasie in die klanke wat voorkom, wel 'rr rol speel behoort die data dit te reflekteer. hierdie moontlikheid word bevestig deur die resultate van die vokaalduurmetings (kyk tabel 4) waar die mate van afwyking by s2 groter is as by si. by die uitingduurmetings (kyk tabel 5) word hierdie moontlikheid egter nie bevestig nie. si toon 'n groter mate van afwyking as s2. dit wil dus voorkom asof die mate van variasie in die klanke in die eenheid nie die oorwegende rede is vir die verhoging in moeilikheidsgraad nie, maar eerder die getal klanke in die eenheid. laasgenoemde afleiding hou egter ook nie volkome stand nie. dit wil voorkom asof die lengte van die uiting nie die enigste faktor is wat die moeilikheidsgraad daarvan bepaal nie want s5 bevat twee klanke meer as s3 maar vertoon feitlik dieselfde die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 a va der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots, r j grimbeek en l ρ c jansen mate van afwyking in vokaalduur (kyk tabel 4) en 'n kleiner mate van afwyking in uitingduur (kyk tabel 5) as s3. die feit dat s5 fonologies meer kompleks is omdat meer klanke geselekteer en gekombineer moet word as by s3, bring dus nie mee dat die afwyking in uitingduur toeneem nie. die verbaal apraktiese spreker neem dus nie baie langer om dit te beplan as 'n uiting met vyf klanke soos in s3 nie. daar is verskillende moontlike verklarings vir die feit dat s3 feitlik dieselfde mate van afwyking vertoon vir vokaalduur en 'n groter mate van afwyking vir uitingduur as s5. tydens die ondersoek is subjektief waargeneem dat s5 meer ritmies geproduseer word deur die verbaal apraktiese sprekers as s3. dit kan wees dat die ritme van 'n eenheid bepaal word deur die klankstruktuur en dat sekere strukture meer ritmies is. keele en summers (1976:139) verklaar dat 'n ritmiese tydstruktuur vaardighede anders be'invloed as 'n arbitrsre tydpatroon. wanneer die tydreeling ritmies is, word die vaardigheid makliker aangeleer. dit word aanvaar dat die basiese eenheid van spraakritme die lettergreep is. klankstruktuur drie bestaan uit twee lettergrepe, maar 'n neiging is waargeneem by die verbaal apraktiese sprekers om die kvkv-gedeelte as 'n eenheid te produseer en die finale /f/-klank as 'n ge'fsoleerde eenheid. dit het moontlik meegebring dat die natuurlike spraakritme versteur is by s3. die versteuring in spraakritme bring dan mee dat die uiting meer afwykend word. 'n verdere moontlike verklaring wat by bogenoemde aansluit, is dat s3 problematies is omdat dit uit 'n kvkv-eenheid bestaan wat 'n duplisering is van die sogenaamde kv-eenheid van spraakprogrammering en 'n addisionele konsonant wat losstaande is. die kvkv-struktuur kom ook soos reeds gemeld vroeg voor in kinderspraak en het waarskynlik beperkte kompleksiteit. dit word dan as 'n eenheid geproduseer deur die sprekers. dit mag ook wees dat lettergreepvorming versteur is, maar dit is on waarskynlik want by s5 was daar geen probleme hiermee nie. by s5 vorm die kvkven die kvk-gedeeltes twee afsondenike eenhede wat deiae met redelike gemak geproduseer kan word terwyl die losstaande klank by s3 moontlik 'n groter mate van beplanning verg. die probleme wat die proefpersone ervaar het met die produksie van s3-eenhede kan moontlik ook die gevolg wees van die spesifieke klank wat in die finale posisie voorkom. die /f/ vereis akkurate aanpassing in lipbeweging en is minder veranderlik as baie ander klanke. die /f/ kom aan die ander kant ook voor in s5 maar moontlik werk die daaropvolgende /u/klank fasiliterend in op die /f/ in hierdie eenhede. die bewegings vir die /f/ in hierdie twee posisies en eenhede verskil. bogenoemde drie moontlike verklarings is die enigste wat voorgestel kan word op grond van die beskikbare data. opsommend kom die resultate daarop neer dat die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting 'n invloed uitoefen op die mate van afwyking in uitingduur. die lengte van die uiting speel 'n belangrike rol. kort uitings word feitlik normaal geproduseer wat die temporale eienskappe vokaalduur en uitingduur betref. die byvoeging van 'n enkele klank bring mee dat produksie meer afwykend is. die lengte van die uiting is egter nie die enigste faktor wat moeilikheidsgraad vir die verbaal apraktiese spreker bepaal nie. die moontlikheid bestaan dat die besondere klankstruktuur van 'n uiting, die inherente ritme daarvan en/ of die klanke wat daarin voorkom, ook 'n rol speel by die moeilikheidsgraad daarvan. die invloed van die artikulasie-eienskappe van 'n uiting op uitingduur. geeneen van die berekende p-waardes is onder 0,0125 nie, wat impliseer dat daar geen betekenisvolle verskille tussen die mate van afwyking in uitingduur van eenhede in die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe is nie. die p-waardes wat verkry is, is egter kleiner as die p-waardes vir vokaalduur. die kleinste waarde vir vokaalduur is 0,4010 terwyl die grootste p-waarde vir uitingduur 0,4202 is. die kleinste waarde is 0,0713 wat vir artikulasie-eienskapgroepe binne si verkry is. numeries is daar ook groot verskille tussen die totaalwaardes vir die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe (kyk tabel 5). dit wil voorkom asof daar tog verskille is tussen die mate waarin die uitingduur van eenhede in verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe afwyk. 'n moontlike verklaring vir die feit dat die invloed op vokaalduur meer beperk is, is waarskynlik dat verlengde vokaalduur slegs 'n gedeelte van die invloed van artikulasie-eienskappe reflekteer. die data oor uitingduur reflekteer die totale invloed en is daarom meer opsigtelik. dit moet in gedagte gehou word dat die artikulasie-eienskappe wat in die eenhede binne 'n struktuurgroep verteenwoordig is, netminimaal verskil. uiting 1.1 en 1.2 en ook 1.3 en 1.4 verskil byvoorbeeld net met 'n enkele klank. dit is dus insiggewend dat hierdie klein verskille in artikulasie-eienskappe wel die groot verskil in totaalwaardes teweegbring. die totale afwyking in millisekondes vir a l is 12554 terwyl dit vir a3 9907 is (kyk tabel 5). die verskille tussen die artikulasie-eienskapgroepe word ook in groot mate gereflekteer in die totale vir die individuele sprekers. dit wil dus voorkom asof die artikulasieeienskappe van 'n uiting wel 'n invloed uitoefen op die duur daarvan en moontlik dus op die moeilikheidsgraad van 'n uiting. gevolgtrekkings daar kan tot die volgende gevolgtrekkings gekom word: interartikulator-sinchronisasie vir korrekte stemgewing is nie sensitief vir die klankstruktuur of artikulasie-eienskappe van 'n uiting nie. stemaanvangstydfoute kom onvoorspelbaar en onkonstant by die herhaling van 'n uiting voor. die gebrek aan konteks-sensitiwiteit en die feit dat nie alle proefpersone stemaanvangstydfoute vertoon nie, dui daarop dat interartikulator-sinchronisasie vir korrekte stemgewing 'n komponent van beplanning is wat onafhanklik van die ander komporiente funksioneer en dat sat-foute 'n kernsimptoom vanyverbale apraksie reflekteer. die sat-foute wat voorkom, is nie ware vervangings met stemlose klanke nie, maar is distorsies weens oorskryding van die kritiese temporale ekwivalensiegrense vir interartikulator-sinchronisasie. variasie in die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting het 'n effek op vokaalduur. vokaalduur is dus konteks-sensitief vir klankstruktuur. namate die moeilikheidsgraad van die uiting toeneem, neem die mate van afwyking in vokaal-. duur ook toe. die verlengde segmentele duur is dus nie 'n direkte gevolg van die neuromotoriese afwyking by die verbaal apraktiese sprekers nie. dit is waarskynlik 'n kompensatoriese strategie om die oriderliggende neuromotoriese probleem te oorkom. sensitiwiteit vir die temporale reel dat vokaalduur verkort voor 'n finale plosief, is behou. die verbaal apraktiese sprekers is dus in staat om temporale aanpassings in die kernmotorprogram te maak the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989, r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur 41 o p g r o n d v a n k e n n i s v a n die b e t r o k k e t e m p o r a l e r e e l e n m e t die d o e i o m te k o m p e n s e e r . variasie i n die a r t i k u l a s i e e i e n s k a p p e w a t in die m a t e r i a a l v e r t e e n w o o r d i g is, h e t n i e ' n s t a t i s t i e s b e d u i d e n d e effek o p v o k a a l d u u r b y die g r o e p v e r b a a l a p r a k t i e s e sprek e r s n i e . d a a r is w e l a a n d u i d i n g s d a t v o k a a l d u u r s e n s i t i e f is vir die a r t i k u l a s i e e i e n s k a p p e v a n ' n u i t i n g . variasie in die k l a n k s t r u k t u u r v a n ' n u i t i n g h e t ' n effek o p die d u u r v a n die u i t i n g . n a m a t e die m o e i l i k h e i d s g r a a d , w a t b e p a a l w o r d d e u r die l e n g t e e n die k l a n k s t r u k t u u r v a n die u i t i n g , t o e n e e m , n e e m die m a t e v a n a f w y k i n g i n u i t i n g d u u r toe. d i e gevolgtrekking d a t v e r l e n g d e d u u r n i e ' n d i r e k t e gevolg v a n die a f w y k i n g is n i e , m a a r ' n k o m p e n s a t o r i e s e strategie, w o r d bevestig. sekere k l a n k s t r u k t u r e ( b y v o o r b e e l d kvkvke e n h e d e ) e n l a n g e r u i t i n g s w o r d verleng, w a a r s k y n l i k o m d a t die b e p l a n n i n g d a a r v a n m e e r k o m p l e k s is. d a a r is a a n d u i d i n g s d a t die a r t i k u l a s i e e i e n s k a p p e v a n ' n u i t i n g ' n invloed uitoefen o p die m a t e v a n a f w y k i n g i n u i t i n g d u u r , m a a r die effek is n i e s t a t i s t i e s b e t e k e n i s v o l b e v i n d n i e . die k o n s e p v a n k o n t e k s s e n s i t i w i t e i t soos o n t l e e n a a n die k o a l i s i e m o d e l h e t d u s o o k m e t b e t r e k k i n g t o t die t e m p o r a l e e i e n s k a p p e v a n v e r b a a l a p r a k t i e s e s p r a a k , die s i m p t o m e i n ' n i n s i g g e w e n d e p e r s p e k t i e f gestel d e u r die o n d e r s k e i d t u s s e n k e r n s i m p t o m e e n g e a s s o s i e e r d e s i m p t o m e a a n te t o o n . verd e r e a a n d u i d i n g s v a n die a a r d v a n die a f w y k i n g is o o k v e r k r y . die o n v e r m o e o m k o n s t a n t die kritiese t e m p o r a l e e k w i v a l e n s i e g r e n s e vir i n t e r a r t i k u l a t o r s i n c h r o n i s a s i e te b e r e i k , is s i g ' b a a r g e d e m o n s t r e e r d e u r die a k o e s t i e s e a n a l i s e v a n herh a a l d e u i t i n g e . die r e s u l t a t e v a n die o n d e r s o e k d u i v e r d e r o o k d a a r o p d a t dieselfde t w e e l e d i g e s i m p t o m a t o l o g i e , a a n g e t r e f w o r d b y verb a l e o n t w i k k e l i n g s a p r a k s i e . e r k e n n i n g s f i n a n s i e l e b y s t a n d v a n die r a a d vir g e e s t e s w e t e n s k a p l i k e n a v o r s i n g vir h i e r d i e o n d e r s o e k , w o r d h i e r m e e e r k e n . v e r w y s i n g s aitken, p.g. en wilson, w.a. discriminative vocal conditioning in rhesus monkeys: evidence for volitional control? brain and language, 8, 227-240, 1979.) blumstein, s.e., cooper, w.e.jzurif, e.b. en caramazza, a. the perception and production of voice-onset time in aphasia. neuropsgcholofita, 15, 371-383, 1977. collins, m., rosenbek, j.c. en wertz, r.t. spectrographic analysis of vowel and word duration .jou rnal of speech and hea ring resea rch, 26, 224-230;?1983. cooper, w.e. the development of speech timing. in s.j. segalowitz en f.a. gruber (reds.) language development and neurological theory. new york: academic press, 1977. di simoni, f.g. influence of consonant environment on duration of vowels in the speech of three-, six and nine-year-old children. journal of the acoustical society of america, 55, 362-363, 1974. duffy, j.r. en gawle, c.a. apraxic speaker's vowel duration in consonant-vowel-consonant syllables. in j.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil en a.e. aronson (reds.) apraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics, management. san diego: college-hill press, 1984. freeman, f.j., sands, e.s. en harris, k.s. temporal coordination of phonation and articulation in a case of verbal apraxia: a voice onset time study. brain and language, 6, 106-111, 1978. hoit-dalgaard, j., murry, t. en kopp, h.g. voice-onset time production and perception in apraxic subjects. brain and language, 20, 329-339, 1983. itoh, m. en sasanuma, s. articulatory movements in apraxia of speech. in j.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil en a.e. aroirton (reds.) appraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics, management. san diego: college-hill press, 1984. jansen, l.p.c. persoonlike onderhoud, forensiese akoestiek-laboratorium van die suid-afrikaanse polisie, pretoria, 1985. jiirgens, u. en ploog, d. on the neural control of mammalian vocalization. trends in neurosciences, 4, 135-137, 1981. jiirgens, u. en von cramon, d. on the role of the anterior cingulate cortex in phonation: a case report. brain and language, 15, 234248, 1982. keele, s. w. en summers, j.j. the structure of motor programs. in g.e. stelmach (red.) motor control: issues and trends. new york: academic press, 1976. kelso, j.a.s. en tuller, b. toward a theory of apractic syndromes. brain and language, 12, 224-245, 1981. kelso, j.a.s., tuller, b. en harris, k.s. a'dynamic pattern' perspective on the control and coordination of movement. in p.f. macneilage (red.) the production of speech. new york: springerverlag, 1983. kent, r.d., netsell, r. en abbs, j.h. acoustic characteristics of dysarthria associated with cerebellar disease. journal of speech and hearing research, 22, 627-648, 1979. kent, r.d. en rosenbek, j.c. acoustic patterns of apraxia of speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 26, 231249, 1983. kewley-port, d. en preston, m.s. early apical stop production: a voice onset time analysis. journal of phonetics, 2, 195-210, 1974. klapp, s.t., anderson, w.g. en berrian, r. w. implicit speech in reading reconsidered. journal of experimental psychology 100, 368374, 1973. kubaska, c.a. en keating, p.a. word duration in early child speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 24, 615621, 1981. marquardt, t.p., reinhart, j.b. en peterson, h.a. markedness analysis of phonemic substitution erors in apraxia of speech. journal of communication disorders, 12, 481-494, 1979. mogeson, g.j., jones, d.l. en yim, c.y. from motivation to action: functional interface between the limbic system and the motor system. progress in neurobiology, 14, 6997, 1980. ploog, d. neurobiology of primate audio-vocal behavior. brain research reviews, 3, 35-61. elsevier: north-holland bio-medical press, 1981. rosenbek, j.c., kent, r.d. en la pointe, l.l. apraxia of speech: an overview and some perspectives. in j.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil en a.e. aronson (reds.)apraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics, management. san diego: college-hill press, 1984. schmidt, r.a. motor control and learning: a behavioral emphasis. illinois: human kinetics publishers, 1982. turvey, m.t., fitch, h.l. en tuller, b. the bernstein perspective: i. the problems of degrees of freedom and context-conditioned variability. in j.a.s. kelso (red.) human motor behavior: an introduction. london: lawrence erlbaum associates, 1982. van der merwe, a. terapieprogram vir verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie met toepassingsmoontlikhede vir ander spraakafwykings. publikasie van die universiteit van pretoria, pretoria: v&r, 1985. van der merwe, a. die motoriese beplanning van spraak by verbale apraksie. ongepubliseerde d.phil-verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria, 1986. van der merwe, α., uys, i.c., loots, j.m. en grimbeek, r.j. die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, 3, 10-22, 1987. van der merwe, α., uys, i.c., loots, j.m. en grimbeek, r.j. ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie: aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 35, 45-54, 1988. wertz, r.t., la pointe, l.l. en rosenbek, j.c. apraxia of speech in adults: the disorder and its management. orlando: grune & stratton, inc., 1984. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 \ r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a legacy of sound advice and equipment philips hearing aids audiometers fm systems head office: 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street p.o. box 3069, johannesburg 2000. tel: (011) 337-7537. philips r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) laryngeal trauma: a diagnostic case study* loren β schneider ba (sp & η thorapyr(witwatersrand) baragwanath hospital, johannesburg anthony traill phd (witwatersrand) department of linguistics university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg lesley wolk ma (speech pathology) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract this study investigates the physiological mechanism responsible for reappearance ofphonation in an aphonic subject with traumatic laryngeal damage. the battery of procedures involved spectrography, laryngographic analysis, fiberoptic laryngoscopy, tomography, lateral xerography and cinefluorography. results show that the subject's abnormal laryngeal anatomy facilitated a novel adductory mechanism for vocal fold vibration involving a type of sphincteric constriction between the arytenoid cartilages and the epiglottis. the value of a battery of diagnostic measures is highlighted. opsomming hierdie studie ondersoek die fisiologiese meganisme wat verantwoordelik is vir die herverskyning vanfonasie in « afonie geval met troumatiese laryngeale beskadiging. die ondersoek battery het spektrografie, laringografiese analise, fiberoptiese laringoskopie, tomografie, laterale xerografie en cinefluorografie ingesluit. resultate dui daarop dat die proefpersoon se abnormale larinks-anatomie 'n ongewone adduksie meganisme bewerkstellig het vir die stemband vibrasie, naamlik, 'n sfinkter vernouing tussen die aritenoide kraakbene en die epiglottis. die waarde van 'n metingsbattery vir diagnose word uitgelig. laryngeal injury was prevalent during the two world wars (luchsinger and arnold, 1965). post-war, the incidence decreased. today, we are again witnessing an acceleration of laryngeal injuries. this has been attributed to the sustained increase in motor vehicle accidents and rise in violent crimes, an inexorable fact of our present society (luchsinger and arnold, 1965). most interest in laryngeal trauma has been confined to medical literature. there is a paucity of research on laryngeal injury with regard to vocal rehabilitation by the speech clinician. a detailed investigation of recent literature reveals only one study viewing laryngeal trauma in relation to voice therapy (mitrinowicz-modrzejewska,j 1962). in this study, two cases of laryngeal trauma are discussed, and it is concluded that voice therapy is possible, even under highly adverse circumstances and following long periods of post-traumatic aphonia. however, differential diagnosis is essential before any treatment is undertaken. trauma results in disorders of variable type and degree, ranging from slight dysphonia to complete aphonia. the nature of the laryngeal damage must be determined, as treatment and prognosis will depend on the site and extent of structural involvement. different perspectives on subjective and objective measures have been highlighted in the literature. it is apparent that earlier clinicians were more dependent on subjective measures, whereas later writers have attempted to objectify evaluation. kelman, gordon, morton and simpson (1981) attribute this to the recent advent of electronic instruments for assessment of vocal behaviour. the advantages of the various objective measures have been emphasized by many writers. the inaccessibility of the phonatory mechanism renders fiberoptic and radiological techniques invaluable in diagnostic evaluation. for acoustic analyses of the speech waveform, writers have stressed the value of spectrography (kerr and lanham, 1973; rontal, rontal and rolnick, .1975) and laryngography (fourcin, 1974). a combination of subjective and objective evaluation is still felt to be optimal (aronson, 1980, p. 171). the major aim of this study was thus to use a multidimensional approach in an in-depth evaluation of the structural and functional components responsible for phonation in a case with laryngeal damage due to trauma. method subject the subject, m.p., is an english-speaking adult male, aged 24 years, who suffered a motorcycle accident in which his larynx was crushed by a wire stretched across the road. summaries of the pertinent post-traumatic medical events are presented in table 1. * this article is based upon the first'author's research report entitled "laryngeal trauma: a diagnostic case study" submitted to the department of speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 sasha 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 loren β schneider, anthony traill and lesley wolk table 1 summary of post-traumatic medical events date event description of findings vocal history 15.01.78 motorcycle accident crushed tracheal cartilages and severe damage to larynx post-traumatic aphonia 30.01.78 tracheostomy; graft inserted around trachea 28.04.78 direct laryngoscopy; bronchoscopy; tracheal dilation left vocal fold moved poorly; right vocal fold normal; marked fibrosis and stenosis 07.05.78 subglottal t-tube insertion vocal cords immobile; ventricular cords moving 08.01.79 removal of t-tube; direct laryngoscopy vocal cords scarred and immobile; ventricular cords moving 01.05.79 direct laryngoscopy; granuloma removal vocal cords immobile; very narrow trachea voice therapy received, resulting in lowpitched, severely hoarse phonation 07.09.81 direct laryngoscopy; laser therapy (for anterior web and subglottal scarring) vocal cords not moving normally; scarring of left vocal cord voice therapy received, resulting in lowpitched, severely hoarse phonation 12.11.82 direct laryngoscopy; laser therapy oedematous vocal cords; some movement detected; ventricular cord closure; no web referred for further voice therapy. the vocal history is as follows: in january 1979 m. p. presented with post-traumatic aphonia. between march and october of that year he received voice therapy resulting in low-pitched, severely hoarse phonation. he reported a gradual improvement in voice quality over the next three years. after the medical examination and laser therapy in november 1982 he was referred for further voice therapy. at this stage it was suggested that nerve re-generation may have occurred to explain movement observed in the vocal cords (see table 1). procedure preliminary measures audiometric evaluation revealed hearing within normal limits bilaterally. oral peripheral examination revealed structural and functional adequacy of the oral peripheral 'mechanism 'for speech. voice evaluation a. subjective evaluation a voice sample was recorded including connected'speech, reading, selected speech sounds and counting at v'avying rates, loudness and pitch levels. aspects of respiration, phonation and resonance were analysed descriptively. a voice scale was completed in an attempt to confirm and objectify subjective evaluation. the buffalo voice profile (wilson, 1979) was selected for evaluation of vocal parameters as a baseline measure and was judged by two qualified speech therapists. the judges were naive as to the aims of the study and had had no previous contact with the subject. b. objective evaluation 1. indirect laryngoscopy indirect laryngoscopy was carried out by an ent specialist following the procedure of mirror laryngoscopy. 2. spectrography spectrographs analysis was performed on a kay sonagraph model 6061-b. broad and narrowband spectrograms were made as well as sections. the vowels la; j and [i:j] were used for analysis and sentences were used to supplement this data. 3. fiberoptic examination \ the flexible fiberoptic laryngoscope provides a means of visualising and identifying normal or abnormal laryngeal structure and regulation during'voice production. aronson (1980) states that this is the-only assessment technique which allows observation of 'the 'vocal folds during connected speech. fiberoptic exantifiatton was carried out, using a flexible fiberoptic nasokiryngoscope olympus type vf4a. several slides were takgn^f^elii7nx^duti0g^quief'fe$i>iration and sustained vowel'prdduction.-phonation'vvas' taipe recorded concurrently,1 allowing for correlation of slides with corresponding utterances. 4. laryngography the laryngograph is "an electrical impedance technique . . . for the direct examination of vocal fold closure, which does not -interfere with phonation" (fourcin, 1974). the laryngographic procedure described by wechsler (1977), was ' carried out, using the fourcin laryngograph and voicescope. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) laryngeal trauma: a diagnostic case study 15 m.p. was required to produce sustained phonation on the vowels [a:] and [i:] during level and rising pitches. a sentence was recorded to supplement the data. the output of the laryngograph and voicescope was simultaneously recorded on a mingograf inkjet recorder. 5. frontal tomography and lateral xerography many writers advocate the use of x-ray techniques in the diagnosis of voice disorders, for example, fletcher, shelton, smith and bosma (1960), who stress the need for research in this area. in traumatic cases, radiological measures aid in determining the extent of structural damage. for an anteroposterior view of the larynx, frontal tomograms were taken. for a lateral view of the larynx, lateral xerography was carried out. tomograms and xerograms were taken at rest and during sustained phonation. the audio signal was tape recorded to ensure that the exposure had coincided with phonation and for later spectrographic analysis. 6. cinefluorography cinefluorography is a technique using "electronic image in. tensification, in which motion pictures are taken from a fluoroscopic screen" (moll, 1960). one of the major advantages of this technique is in its application to the dynamics of connected speech. this allows the study of movement, rather than a single pose taken at one instant in time. a barium compound was introduced through the subject's nostrils into the pharynx to add clarity to the image, and he produced a few seconds of spontaneous speech and sustained vowel phonation to determine the exact locus and nature of his vibratory source. results subjective evaluation table 2 represents a summary of the parameters evaluated qualitatively by the investigators, from a recorded speech sample, and includes findings from the rated buffalo voice profiles. / / khz 3,0 η 2,5 2,0 1,0 οβ -\ table 2 summary of findings of subjective voice analysis respiration thoracic type of breathing; restricted capacity; poor respiratory control, as assessed through phono-respiratory tasks extracted from boone (1977, p. 85) loudness adequate volume in habitual phonation, but progressive decrease in loudness during sustained vowel production, which could be related to inadequate respiratory function pitch low habitual and optimum pitch levels; limited pitch range; no pitch breaks. quality hoarse; breathy; auditorily rough; diplophonic steadiness steady at habitual pitch level; unsteady when required to raise pitch level, possibly attributable to excessive tension. rate within normal limits, confirmed by raters (wilson, 1979, p. 88) resonance within expected range, confirmed by raters (boone, 1977, p. 98) overall vocal efficiency moderately impaired objective evaluation a. indirect laryngoscopy examination during quiet breathing revealed no significant variables in terms of the medical status of μ. p.'s laryngeal structures. assymmetry of the vocal cords was reported, with the left vocal cord situated at a higher level than the right vocal cord. during phonation a posterior movement of the epiglottis obstructed the view of the vocal cords, preventing any observation of their state during phonation. figure 1 narrow band spectrograms and acoustic sections of the sustained vowels [i:] and [a:]. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 loren β schneider, anthony traill and lesley wolk b. spectrographs analysis the broadband and narrowband spectrograms were analysed in terms of the following parameters: 1. fundamental frequency the fundamental frequency was measured at various places in connected speech. the figures obtained were averaged, yielding a fundamental frequency of 77 hz. this is well below the norm for males, i.e. 120 hz. (fry, 1979). 2. harmonic components in comparison to a normal spectrogram a relatively restricted range of harmonics was evident. as is illustrated in figure 1, in some parts of the narrowband spectrograms and on the sections, clear harmonics were evident with a good signal to noise ratio, while in others the signal to noise ratio was poor. nevertheless, during production of both [a:] and [i:] a sufficiently rich range of harmonics was always evident. 3. formant structure in normal phonation, the first four formants are usually clear (kerr and lanham, 1973). in m.p.'s case, the first three formants were present on most of the wideband spectrograms. however, the higher formants were noise excited. rontal et al (1975) state that clear formants depend on a lack of breathiness, normal periodicity and a good resonating system. despite the presence of breathiness and aperiodicity in m.p.'s voice, subjective analysis suggested that the resonatory system was adequate. 4. pitch range m.p. was able to achieve limited pitch variation of 34 hz. when asked to produce vowels on a series of pitches, from lowest through to highest. the highest and second highest pitched vowels he produced had a greater number of clear harmonics than the three lower pitched vowels, indicating that optimal vibratory adjustment occurred at a higher pitch. however, it required additional effort and discomfort for m.p. to raise his pitch to these levels. c. laryngographic analysis the parameters analysed were as follows: 1. lx: lx provides information concerning the mode of vibration of the vocal folds, and excludes supraglottal information. the following aspects of lx were assessed: a) regularity: in the normal larynx, the lx pattern is regularly repeated, through control of lung air pressure and laryngeal musculature (abberton, 1972). as can be seen in figure 2, m.p.'s lx behaviour was complex and highly irregular throughout the laryngogram. by contrast, the normal lx waveform represented in figure 3 is simple and completely regular, indicating a stable vibratory source. m.p.'s lx tracings exhibit gross cycle to cycle waveform changes, a condition which may be described (with a little licence) as "lx shimmer", indicating abnormal vocal fold vibration. b) shape: abberton (1972) describes three phases in the shape of a normal vibratory cycle: — a rapid closing phase (positive going), i.e. a rise in the waveform associated with the interval of greatest acoustic excitation of the vocal tract. — a slower opening phase (negative going), i.e. a decline in the waveform associated with the gradual parting of the vocal folds, as the subglottal pressure increases prior to abduction. — an open phase, i.e. a flat base corresponding to the interval when the vocal folds are out of contact and the glottis is abducted. the shape of m.p.'s lx waveform is thoroughly irregular in all these phases. given the abnormal anatomy of this case it would be unwise to speculate on the precise physiological correlates of the irregularities. however, it may safely be said that the lx tracings provide clear evidence of variability in the mass, adduction, abduction and probably tension of the vibratory source. 2. fx: fx is a display of fundamental frequency. as illustrated in the normal case in figure 3, fx takes the form of an even or smoothly varying line. throughout m.p.'s fx tracing, an abnormality, termed "jitter", was evident, i.e. small cycle to cycle pitch perturbations due to aperiodicity of the vibratory mechanism. figure 2 illustrates this. the amount of jitter and shimmer in a signal is directly related to the listener's perceptions of auditory roughness (wendahl, 1966) and in this case, the large amount of jitter and shimmer corresponded to the rough, hoarse voice quality evident in subjective evaluation. i fx lx figure 2 m.p. 's lx and fx tracings during the production of [i:]. the irregularity of the former would be produced by a highly variable vibratory source. the lack of smoothness of the fx trace reflects jitter. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) laryngeal trauma: a diagnostic case study 17 fx —• ^ ft, ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft 'v ft ft ft ft ^ ft ft ft ft ft ft ft ft . >i \ \ \ \ · \ \ l > v \ \ v v \ v v : ·. \ : \ . \ ; ν ; \ \ : '< > \ \ \ · \ v v 1 \ ι i; " η ^ ^ ν η l ί u ν η ν v v η v i u u ι u ι ^ figure 3 normal lx waveform of an adult male. the simple and regular lx trace and the smooth fx trace contrast with those in figure 2. gx and lx \ • \ j rt j f^j l\ ft s a / \ w w ι gx and lx / ίκλ/' ν figure 4 two laryngographic tracings illustrating gx movements frequently observed prior to the onset of phonation (marked with an arrow). 3. gx: slower changes on the lx tracing are termed gx and they are primarily the result of gross movements of the entire larynx before, during and after phonation, brought about by the function of the extrinsic muscles. figure 4 illustrates the large gx movements that were frequently observable prior to the onset of phonation by m.p. although he does not have a normal laryngeal structure, these gx movements are precisely what could be expected from the independently observed laryngeal airway adjustments seen during lateral xerography and cinefluorography (see figure 4). d. fiberoptic analysis slides taken during fiberoptic examination were analysed descriptively at rest and during phonation, in order to determine the structural changes evident during phonation: 1. at rest figure 5 shows a clear view of one true vocal fold, both ventricular folds, the tips of the arytenoid cartilages and the cushion of the epiglottis. one ventricular fold is larger than the other, approximating the midline and obscuring one true vocal fold. slight asymmetry of the arytenoid cardie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 loren β schneider, anthony traill and lesley wolk tilages in relation to each other is evident. during examination, the left arytenoid and vocal fold appeared to be situated at a higher level than the corresponding structures on the right, confirming the findings of indirect laryngoscopy. figure 5 fiberoptic view of m.p. 's larynx taken at rest. the tips of the arytenoids are visible in the top part of the picture, one true fold and the two ventricular folds in the middle and the cushion of the epiglottis at the bottom centre. 2 during phonation of fi:] it was never possible to view the vocal folds during phonation as the rim of the epiglottis moved posteriorly, obscuring the view of the glottis, as is evident in figure 6. the posterior aspect of the arytenoids is visible below the epiglottis. during the examination, no vibration of the posterior aspect of the arytenoids was apparent during phonation. figure 6 fiberoptic view of m.p. 's larynx taken during phonation of [i:]. the rim of the epiglottis is visible in the foreground. in the background the posterior aspect of the arytenoid cartilages can be seen. the dark spot at the centre of the picture corresponds to the junction between the arytenoids. e. tomography because trauma and surgery had distorted m.p.'s laryngeal structures, conventional landmarks were missing, making it extremely difficult to estimate section depths for the tomograms. although tomograms were taken at several section depths, clarity did not result and it was not possible to reach any conclusions about the position of the vocal folds during phonation. f. lateral xerography xerograms selected for analysis were those taken at rest and during phonation. xerograms were analysed using overlay tracings, with bony landmarks as reference points. the cervical vertebrae were used as orientation points by which to align the tracings. in this case conventional grid measures were inapplicable because of the abnormal anatomy, for example, absence of the cricoid and thyroid cartilages. thus, anatomical changes were analysed descriptively, under the following parameters, selected from a list provided by berry, epstein, fourchin, freeman, maccurtain and noscoe (1982). the reader should consult figure 7 during the following discussion. structural changes between quiet respiration (solid lines) and phonation (dotted lines) in m.p. 's larynx. ι 1. epiglottis one of the major physiological changes evident between quiet respiration and phonation, was a pronounced and abnormal anterior rotation of the base of the epiglottis and a posterior tilting of its rim. although the mechanism underlying this movement cannot be stated precisely, it appears to be facilitated by the absence of the thyroid cartilage, which would normally obstruct any anterior movementof the root of the epiglottis. ' 2. hyoid bone during phonation, there was a marked displacement of the hyoid bone anteriorly. despite this anterior movement, the angle of the hyoid bone is maintained. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 31 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) laryngeal trauma: a diagnostic case study 19 3. hypopharynx another major physiological change was an anterior and upward movement of the arytenoid cartilages towards the epiglottis, resulting in a marked expansion of the hypopharynx, both antero-posteriorly and supero-inferiorly. 4. vocal folds although the vocal folds were not clearly visible on the lateral xerograms, their presence could be inferred partly from the shape of laryngeal ventricles which were relatively distinct. two ventricles were visible, one directly above the other, suggesting that the true vocal folds were lying at different heights. this was consistent with superior displacement of the left vocal fold evident during indirect laryngoscopy and fiberoptic examination. during phonation these ventricles were elongated slightly as they moved anteriorly and slightly superiorly in consort with similar movements of the hyoid bone and arytenoid cartilages. it seems reasonable to assume, therefore, that the vocal folds also moved anteriorly and superiorly during adduction. 5. dimensions of the laryngeal airway anterior expansion of the entire laryngeal airway was evident during phonation, again in consort with the anterior movement of the other laryngeal structures mentioned. subglottally, a posterior expansion of the airway was also evident, probably due to air-pressure build-up during phonation. the most interesting physiological change from the rest position to phonation involved the articulation of the arytenoid structure relatively high up on the epiglottis. it is well known that generalisations about the precise physiology of phonation in connected speech cannot be made from lateral xerography, as this technique provides a static view. because of this m.p.'s phonatory mechanism was investigated using cinefluorography. g. cinefluorography the lateral cinefluorographioview of the larynx confirmed the postures evident on the lateral xerograms, i.e. anterior rotation of the base of the epiglottis from a vertical position at rest, through about 45° during phonation. from lateral xerography it was suspected that vibration between the rotated epiglottis and the arytenoids could have: occurred. however, the dynamic image provided by cinefluorography revealed no vibration between these structures. ! 1 / discussion the result of the xerographic and cinefluorographic investigations revealed that a number of unusual physiological adjustments were involved in m.p.'s phonatory mechanism. these included the extreme tilting of the epiglottis, the prominent expansion of the supraand subglottic airway and the hypopharynx, and the articulation of the arytenoids relatively high on the broad part of the epiglottis all undoubtedly due to the subject's abnormal anatomy. in the normal case the base of the epiglottis is attached to the inner aspect of the angle of the thyroid cartilage by the thyro-epiglottic ligament. in m.p.'s case the thyroid cartilage is missing, leaving the base of the epiglottis free to move in the anterior direction seen in figure 7. this in turn allows for an unusual rotation of the epiglottis relative to the arytenoid cartilages allowing them to articulate high up rather than at the level of the tubercle. the lack of cartilaginous support and the surgically architected airway dimensions are responsible for the expansions in the airway both suband supraglotally. the position adopted by the epiglottis during phonation prevented any laryngoscopic observation of the vibratory source. this emphasizes the contribution of x-ray techniques. however, it was possible to establish from fiberoptic examination that neither the epiglottis nor the posterior aspects of the arytenoids vibrated during phonation. it should be mentioned that this position of the epiglottis represented a clear physiological innovation in m.p.'s adductory mechanism that arose subsequent to the situation in 1979. earlier it was indeed possible to see into the larynx during phonation and to observe a narrowing of the laryngeal sphincter during attempts at phonation. the sequence of events suggests that the rotation of the epiglottis is directly involved in some way with the improved phonatory mechanism that developed. speculations as to what this role might be will be discussed at a later stage. while the x-ray data provided an invaluable perspective on these unusual physiological changes, they did not provide much useful information about the role of the vocal cords during phonation. at best they showed that the location of the cords changed during phonation and that they were probably elongated slightly as they shifted. whether the cords actually vibrated and, if so, in what manner, had to be investigated by other means, namely, spectrographically and laryngographically. the lx and fx tracings provide complementary information on the nature of the vibratory source. briefly, they show that the vibrations are irregular in terms of their periodicity and waveform. this permits the inference that there is no fine control over the vibratory source so far as bulk and tension are concerned. the complex and variable waveform shapes present a unique picture with double peaking on either the closing or opening phase. no straightforward physiological interpretation of these complexities is possible because of m.p.'s abnormal anatomy. however, double peaking normally suggests that more than one structure may be involved in the vibratory source and that these are variably present in adduction and abduction. typical candidates for this sort of complexity are the vocal cords together with the ventricular folds. in m.p.'s case this could be so, with the height discrepancies between the vocal cords adding yet another "level" to the structures involved in adduction. another structure that prima facie could be involved is the aryepiglottic adduction mechanism. indeed, the x-ray data may be readily interpreted as showing that m.p. was simply using a well-known site for the development of a pseudo-glottis (luchsinger and arnold, 1965). however, apart from the unusually high position of the adduction, this possibility does not seem likely. first, if the vibratory source is located at this ary-epiglottic sphincter, it is difficult to account for the complete lack of visible vibration of either part. neither the arytenoids nor the epiglottis could be seen to vibrate during either fiberoptic or cinefluorographic investigation. second, the location of the electrodes was not sufficiently high on the neck to detect impedance changes at the level of m.p.'s arydie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 loren β schneider, anthony traill and lesley wolk epiglottic sphincter. third, the spectrographic evidence strongly suggests that m.p.'s vibratory source was unlikely to involve the arytenoid cartilages or epiglottis. this evidence concerns the rich harmonic structure best seen on the acoustic sections in figure 1. the large number of modes of vibration (i.e. harmonics) most strongly suggests that the vocal cords are dominantly involved in m.p.'s vibratory source. there are no other laryngeal structures capable of vibrating in that many modes. while this vibratory behaviour is less than optimal, involving aperiodicity and noise, it is, nevertheless, the best evidence we have that m.p.'s cords retain the potential for complex harmonic motion. there is no independent evidence that the false cords are involved and so the double peaking on the lx waveform can be attributed to the uneven levels of the cords relative to one another and to the resulting asymmetrical adductory possibilities. we now return to the role of the physiological changes seen in figure 7. the question arises as to why these unusual supraglottal adjustments are necessary if m.p. is indeed using his true vocal cords for phonation. we suggest that they constitute m.p.'s adductory mechanism for the vocal cords in the absence of some of the parts of the normal mechanism. the fact that the changes are a pre-requisite for phonation, together with the observation that they directly affect the position of the cords on the xerograms suggests this is so. as to why the adduction mechanism should, in a sense, be so elaborate, we can only speculate that it represents a good example of transferred function in a larynx with extensive structural damage. m.p.'s control of the laryngeal sphincter is obviously not impaired and he has learnt to exercise a fine control over this highly efficient adductory mechanism in order to create conditions for vocal cord vibration. the findings of the measures carried out on this case cannot be generalised to other cases of laryngeal trauma. however, some comments concerning the use of a battery of diagnostic measures are apposite. no one measure used in this study provides a complete diagnosis on its own. rather, the value of each measure is highlighted, within the context of the battery as a whole. in evaluation of this subject, the objective analyses revealed acoustic displays of vocal parameters and the physiological mechanism responsible for phonation, and subjective analysis revealed the auditory quality of the phonation being produced by this mechanism. when m.p. was referred for further voice therapy at the end of 1982, it was clinically impossible to determine whether further therapy was indicated, and if so the nature of this therapy. it was only after a full battery of diagnostic measures was carried out, that it was possible to prognosticate. results of the investigation indicated that m.p. was in fact using the optimal phonation possible with his damaged laryngeal mechanism; a final goal for therapy (luchsinger and arnold, 1965). hence, no futher voice therapy was indicated. in conclusion, a battery of diagnostic measures (both subjective and objective measures) appears to be most valuable in the rehabilitative process of patients with vocal disorders. a dearth of research on laryngeal trauma within the realm of speech pathology exists. it is hoped that this study will promote further interest in this area. acknowledgement dr. m. said's generous assistance with the cinefluorography is gratefully acknowledged. references abberton, e. some laryngographic data for korean stops, j. int. phonetic assoc., 2, 67-78, 1972. aronson, a.e. clinical voice disorders, brian c. decker, new york, 1980. berry, r.j., epstein, r., fourcin, a.j., freeman, m., maccurtain, f. and noscoe, n. an objective analysis of voice disorders: part one, br. j. disord. commun., 17, 67-76, 1982. boone, d.r. the voice and voice therapy, prentice-hall inc., englewood cliffs, new jersey, 1977. fletcher, s.g., shelton, r.l., smith, c.c. and bosma, j.f. radiography in speech pathology, j. speech. hear. dis., 25, 135-144, 1960. fourcin, a.j. laryngoscopic examination of vocal fold vibration, in ventilatory and phonatory control systems, wyke, b. (ed.), oxford university press, london, 315-326. 1974. fry, d.b. the physics of speech, cambridge university press, 1979. kelman, a.w., gordon, m.t., morton, f.m. and simpson, i.c. comparison of methods for assessing vocal function, folia phoniatr., 33, 51-65, 1981. kerr, w. a. and lanham, l.w. anatomical and spectrographic analysis of the voice in disease: a report of five cases, journal of the south african speech and hearing association., 20, 81-107, 1973. luchsinger, r. and arnold, g.e. voice speech language, wadsworth publishing co., belmont, ca., 1965. 1 mitrinowicz-modrezjewska, v.a. posttraumatische stiminstorungen und ihre phoniatrische behandlung, folia phoniatr., 15, 15-22, 1962. i moll, k.l. cinefluorographic techniques in speech research, j. speech hear. res., 227-241, 1960. | rontal, c., rontal, m. and rolnick, m.i. objective evaluation of voice pathology using voice spectrography, ann. otol, rhinol. and laryngol., 84, 662-671, 1975. wechsler, e. laryngographic study of voice disorders, br. j. disord. commun., 12 , 9-22, 1977. wendahl, r.w. some parameters of auditory roughness, folia phoniatr., 18, 26-32, 1966. wilson, d.k. voice problems of children, the williams and wilkins co., baltimore, 1979. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) clark early language programme * carrow auditory-visual abilities test * communication training programme * fokes sentence builder * fokes sentence builder expansion * fokes written language programme * auditory discrimination in depth * developmental language lessons * developmental language stories * language rehabilitation programme * universal articulation programme * w o o d c o c k language proficiency battery * test for auditory * carrow elicited language cards * parts of speech * pea token test for children * trol therapy * remediation harty preschool speech and comprehension of language inventory * tr large picture ce-harmony-awareness * the personalized fluency conof vocal hoarseness * flulanguage screening test * here? posters * voice disoru e t h r u s t colour a n d shape posters * what's wrong here? posters * voice disorders * t h r u s t therapy * ν θ · n i l · π ί ! ΐ ι · π ϊ ΐ 1 · ι ί ΐ ] ^ β 0 α β υ ι α ρ υ c omprehensib k i t e s b j i m m i i i i j m o n scale • lindamood a b ^ ^ j μ · · · · · · · · · ^ β υ β ι τ ο ρ υ con ceptualization test * developmental syntax programme * weiss comprer e a d (pty) ltd. educational resources hensive a r t i c u l a t i o n ! a cognitive-linguist i tegy * the syntax gam γ velopmental scale * cards * pictures for language programm visual abilities test ' i ning programme * fok | fokes sentence b u i l d · written language pr indiscrimination in dep language lessons * guage stories * langu programme * univer test * infant learning ic intervention strae * birth to three des e q u e n c e p i c t u r e sounds * clark early ε * carrow auditoryicommunication trai-es sentence builder * er expansion * fokes ogramme 4 auditory ' ύ η * d e v e l o p m e n t a l developmental lana g e r e h a b i l i t a t i o n sal articulation programme * woodcock language proficiency battery * test for auditory age * r . a r r d ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ m ^ l ^ μ β β 1 e l i c i t e d language inventory * tr large picture cards ' parts of speech * peaceh a r m o n y a w ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ h w ^ i j i f t z t ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ h a r e n e s s t o k e n t c z t ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ m k ^ ^ ^ w f o r c h i l d ren * personalized fluency control therapy · remediation of vocal h o a r s e n e s s ^ ^ ^ ^ r . j ι i t j f l u h a r t y p h f f i p h n n i s mi j • a n d language screening test * colour and shape posters * what's wrong here? posters * voice disorders ' tongue thrust therapy * vocabulary comprehension scale * lindamood auditory conceptualization test * dev e l o p m e n t a ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ j ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ j ^ ^ ^ ^ j ^ w p ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ l syntax pro gramme * comprehensive a r t i ^ h h f l y k p n m u t e st * infant l ^ ^ u u e m i u l u a c o g n i t i v e l i ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ q ^ ^ ^ ^ k h m a l j u l ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ l n g u i s t i c intervention strategy * the syntax game * birth to three developmental scale sequency picture cards λ pictures for sounds * clark early die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abstract introduction methods results ethical considerations discussion conclusion acknowledgements references about the author(s) anél botha department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa elizbé ras department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa shabnam abdoola department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa jeannie van der linde department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa citation botha, a., ras, e., abdoola, s., & van der linde, j. (2017). dysphonia in adults with developmental stuttering: a descriptive study. south african journal of communication disorders 64(1), a347. https://doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v64i1.347 original research dysphonia in adults with developmental stuttering: a descriptive study anél botha, elizbé ras, shabnam abdoola, jeannie van der linde received: 10 nov. 2016; accepted: 20 mar. 2017; published: 26 june 2017 copyright: © 2017. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract background: persons with stuttering (pws) often present with other co-occurring conditions. the world health organization’s (who) international classification of functioning, disability and health (icf) proposes that it is important to understand the full burden of a health condition. a few studies have explored voice problems among pws, and the characteristics of voices of pws are relatively unknown. the importance of conducting future research has been emphasised. objectives: this study aimed to describe the vocal characteristics of pws. method: acoustic and perceptual data were collected during a comprehensive voice assessment. the severity of stuttering was also determined. correlations between the stuttering severity instrument (ssi) and the acoustic measurements were evaluated to determine the significance. twenty participants were tested for this study. result: only two participants (10%) obtained a positive dysphonia severity index (dsi) score of 1.6 or higher, indicating that no dysphonia was present, while 90% of participants (n = 18) scored lower than 1.6, indicating that those participants presented with dysphonia. some participants presented with weakness (asthenia) of voice (35%), while 65% presented with a slightly strained voice quality. moderately positive correlations between breathiness and ssi (r = 0.40, p = 0.08) have been reported. in addition, participants with high ssi scores also scored a poor dsi of below 1.6, as observed by a moderate positive correlation between ssi and dsi (r = 0.41). conclusion: the majority of pws presented with dysphonia, evident in the perceptual or acoustic parameters of their voices. these results can be used for further investigation to create awareness and to establish intervention strategies for voice disorders among pws. introduction developmental stuttering (ds) is the most common type of stuttering, affecting approximately 1% of the adult population (watkins, smith, davis & howell, 2008). it is also a complex and multifactorial disorder as many different factors play a role (guitar, 2014). more specifically ds is related to abnormalities within the speech planning and production and auditory feedback during speech (ingham et al., 2004). during stuttering, there is abnormal functioning of the whole speech system, including the larynx (salihović, junuzović-žunić, ibrahimagić & beganović, 2009). abnormal functioning of the larynx may include excessive muscular tension and variable subglottal pressure, which could be caused by muscle incoordination of the respiratory tract. weaker laryngeal neuromuscular control and disturbances in respiratory and laryngeal control may also lead to voice problems (salihović et al., 2009). persons with stuttering (pws) often present with other co-occurring conditions such as anxiety, a low self-esteem and negative responses to their communication partners (erickson & block, 2013). these co-occurring conditions may complicate the communication process and affect the emotional well-being and quality of life of pws negatively (blood, blood, maloney, meyer & qualls, 2007). an individual’s feelings and attitudes can be as much part of the disorder of stuttering as his speech behaviours. at first, stuttering may result in unnoticeable, repetitive stuttering behaviours. as stuttering increases, feelings of frustration and shame may also increase, which result in tense and effortful speech that impede fluency (guitar, 2014). pws may have negative attitudes about themselves, which are derived from years of stuttering experiences, and they often project these attitudes on listeners. the relationship between stuttering and emotions varies among individuals who stutter. for some individuals, emotions may contribute to the aetiology of stuttering and for others, stuttering may evoke emotions such as frustration, fear and anger (guitar, 2014). a basic and powerful way to express one’s emotions and to convey messages is through the voice (frühholz, trost & grandjean, 2014). the human voice has been described as an embodiment of self in a social context, contributing to expression, perception and mutual exchange of self, consciousness, inner life and personhood (sidtis & kreiman, 2011). if pws present with negative emotions or anxiety as a result of different factors, including stuttering, it may negatively impact the voice characteristics, such as voice quality (pullin & cook, 2013). voice problems in pws may occur because of attempts to mask their stuttering by changing their pitch or volume to an inappropriate level, leading to a misuse in voice (cooper, 1979). yet, only cooper (1979) and salihović et al. (2009) have conducted studies to explore the voice quality of pws. however, few studies have explored the relationship between dysphonia and pws in the past. the world health organization’s (who) international classification of functioning, disability and health (icf) proposes that it is important to understand the full burden of a health condition. to determine the impact of the condition, information about the disorder and how it impacts the functioning of an individual needs to be determined. because only a few studies have explored voice problems in pws, the importance of conducting future research has been emphasised (salihović et al., 2009). from the gap in existing literature and the multimodality of stuttering, as well as the effect of stuttering on all the different areas of an individual’s life, the following research question is posed: what are the vocal characteristics of adults diagnosed with a developmental stutter? methods aim the study aimed to describe the vocal characteristics of adults with ds. setting and participants the study was conducted at the department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria. a total of 20 adults who were able to speak either afrikaans or english as first or second language were selected to take part in the study. inclusion criteria required the participants to exhibit with a ds only, with no other co-occurring communication disorders. individuals who had any known speech, language, hearing disorders or syndromes, apart from stuttering, were excluded from the study. the participants had to be between the age of 18 and 60 years. anatomical and physiological changes of the vocal folds occur as individuals age and affect the acoustic output of the voice (xue & deliyski, 2001). therefore, participants over the age of 60 were excluded from this study. participants also had to be non-smokers as smoking has an evident effect on some acoustic voice parameters (banjara, mungutwar, singh & gupta, 2014). description of participants case histories indicated that all 20 participants were diagnosed with ds during their preschool years and have previously received or are still receiving stuttering therapy. none of the participants have been diagnosed with a voice disorder or are receiving voice therapy. the study included 7 (35%) women and 13 (65%) men. the average age of participants were 23.5 years (s.d. = 4.95). the home language distribution included 30% (n = 6) english, 25% (n = 5) afrikaans, 20% (n = 4) sepedi, 10% (n = 2) setswana and isizulu and 5% (n = 1) sesotho speakers. approximately a third (35%) of the participants reported to have an average daily intake of carbonated drinks of one to two cups per day and 10% an average intake of three to four cups per day. a few participants (10%) reported an alcohol consumption of one to two glasses per week, and only 5% indicated that they consume three to four glasses per week. only one participant reported to take anti-histamines for allergy-related conditions, and one participant zalascopyrin and celebrex for juvenile arthritis. five participants (25%) indicated that they regularly experienced symptoms of sinusitis. one participant (5%) reported endocrine illness and one (5%) presented with oesophageal reflux. participant 4 and participant 19 indicated that they had experienced a change in their voice quality over time, with participant 4 experiencing a weakness in the voice and participant 19 experiencing hoarseness. however, no voice problems were diagnosed in any of the participants, and they were not receiving treatment for their voice symptoms. voice and speech assessment protocol background questionnaire the background questionnaire consisted of questions about the participant’s background history as well as questions related to their voice and stuttering experiences. demographic information such as the participant’s language, race and marital status was also obtained through the questionnaire. the questionnaire used in the study by van wyk et al. (2016) was used as a guide to the questions used. acoustic voice analysis the computerized speech lab 4300 hardware system was used for the analysis and feedback of acoustic measurements. a comprehensive voice analysis was conducted by using the multi-dimensional voice program and the voice range profile software packages (kaypentax, 2008) to evaluate the voice quality of the participants. the parameters that were assessed included the jitter, shimmer, highest frequency, lowest intensity, noise-to-harmonics ratio and the fundamental frequency values of a phonated/a/sound (table 1). the voice range profile displays the vocal intensity range versus fundamental frequency (f0). table 1: description of acoustic parameters. the dysphonia severity index (dsi) is an index used to create an objective and quantitative link of the perceived voice quality. the dsi is an objective measure as no perceptual analysis is necessary to determine the index. the dsi ranges from +5 (i.e. no dysphonia are present) to -5 (i.e. a severely dysphonic voice). the smaller the dsi, the greater the severity of the dysphonia. while normal voice quality is evident when a positive dsi (1.6 and higher) is obtained (wuyts et al., 2000), the voice measurements that are used to calculate the dsi include the lowest intensity (i-low in db), highest frequency (f0-high in hz), maximum phonation time (mpt in seconds) and the jitter (%), (see equation 1): perceptual voice and speech analysis the perceptual voice and speech analysis of the clients was conducted through the analysis of a spontaneous speech sample and a recorded speech sample of the participants’ voices. the recordings were obtained through the participants who read either an afrikaans or english passage [na die wildtuin/rainbow passage (fairbanks, 1960)]. the instruments that were used included the grade of hoarseness, roughness, breathiness, asthenia, strain and instability (grbasi) 4-point scale (yamauchi, imaizumi, maruyama & haji, 2010) and the stuttering severity instrument (ssi-4; riley, 2009). the grbasi perceptual rating scale was recommended by the japanese society of logopedics and phoniatrics and the european research group for the use in clinical and research settings (yamauchi et al., 2010). the ssi-4 was used to determine the frequency, duration, physical concomitants and the severity of stuttering. based on these parameters, the ssi was determined as very mild, mild, moderate, severe or very severe (riley, 2009). a speech sample that was recorded during the voice assessment was used to determine the severity of stuttering. the ratings were performed after the voice assessment. a listener panel, consisting of three listeners, took part in the perceptual analysis. to increase the reliability and validity of the perceptual analysis, a blind rating was performed by the three listeners. all three listeners had normal hearing and were student researchers. voice handicap index the voice handicap index (vhi) measures the influence of voice problems on a patient’s quality of life. (maertens & de jong, 2007). the vhi is a 30-item self-administered questionnaire (subjective rating scale) that required from the participants to describe their voice and the effects of their voice on their life. the vhi consists of three subscales, which cover the areas of functional, emotional and physical aspects of voice disorders. the vhi was scored after the assessment by the researchers. the overall score was interpreted as mild, moderate or severe. data analysis: all data were analysed by means of descriptive statistics by using statistical software (stata). correlations between the ssi and the acoustic measurements, including the strength and direction (negative or positive) of a relationship between two variables, were evaluated using spearman’s rank correlation coefficients. correlations between the variables of 0.2 ≤ 0.39 were classified weak; 0.4 ≤ 0.59 were moderate; 0.6 ≤ 0.79 were strong; and 0.8 ≤ 1.0 were very strong (mukaka, 2012). pearson’s correlation coefficients were calculated between normal distributed variables, and the non-parametric spearman’s rank correlation coefficients for correlation between the non-normal variables. spearman’s rank and, where appropriate, point bi-serial correlation values were determined between ssi, dsi, vhi and the demographic variables. chi-squared tests were used to identify significant associations at 5% and 10% significance level between the categorical demographic variables, grbasi scores and ssi categories. results the stuttering severity of participants (n = 20) is depicted in table 2. the majority, 75% (n = 15), of participants presented with a very mild stutter, while only 10% (n = 2) presented with a severe stutter. the type of stuttering observed in participants consisted of syllable repetitions, 70% (n = 14), prolongations, 55% (n = 11), blocks, 55% (n = 11), and avoidance of words, 40% (n = 8). the estimated duration of the blocks in 40% (n = 8) of the participants was between 2 and 9 s, one full second in 25% (n = 5) of the participants and the remaining 35% (n = 7) presented with estimated duration of less than 1 s. no physical concomitants were observed in 65% (n = 13) of participants during moments of stuttering. however, very distracting sounds were observed in 15% (n = 3) of participants, distracting facial grimaces in 20% (n = 4) of participants and very distracting head movements in 20% (n = 4) of the participants. table 2: description of stuttering severity of participants (n = 20) according to the stuttering severity instrument. the vocal characteristics of the participants are presented according to perceptual analysis of voice, acoustic voice analysis and self-rating on the vhi. the consensus score across the grbasi scale (table 3) indicated that the perceptual results of many of the participants deviate from normal voice quality. some participants presented with a slight weakness (asthenia) of voice (35%), while the majority of participants (65%) presented with a slightly strained voice quality. table 3: consensus scores of participants (n = 20) across grade of hoarseness, roughness, breathiness, asthenia, strain and instability scale. in table 4 the results of the acoustic voice analysis are presented. the median and interquartile ranges (iqrs) are compared to the expected norms for each acoustic outcome and the correlations between them. table 4: acoustic analysis: descriptive statistics and norms. the jitter (p < 0.001), maximum frequency (p < 0.001), minimum intensity (p < 0.001), mpt (p = 0.001) and the dsi (p < 0.001) all presented with significant differences between the recorded medians and the norms. the shimmer, minimum frequency, maximum intensity and the s–z ratio are the only measurements whose medians fall within the norm or are relatively close to the norm. the median minimum intensity (65 db) is significantly louder than the norm of 40 db. the mpt is significantly lower than the norm of 20 s, indicating that most participants were not able to phonate the/a/sound for 20 s or more. only 15% of the participants were able to sustain phonation of the/a/sound for 20 s or more. the dsi results also indicated a significantly lower score when compared to the norm, indicating a moderate dysphonia in the participants. only two participants (10%) obtained a dsi score of 1.6 or higher while 90% (n = 18) of participants scored lower than 1.6. according to the self-rating of the vhi, 40% (n = 8) of the participants scored between 0 and 30 on the vhi, indicating a low level score. the majority of participants, 55% (n = 11), scored between 31 and 60, indicating a moderate level of handicap and only 5% (n = 1) greater than 60 points, indicating a severe level of handicap. the statements shown in table 5 were extracted from the vhi and represent the statements that received the highest scores from participants. table 5: voice handicap index statements with highest self-rating scores by participants (n = 20). in this study, participants with high ssi scores, also scored a poor dsi of below 1.6, as observed by a moderate positive correlation between ssi and dsi (r = 0.41). although the correlations given in table 6 are not all statistically significant, the directions of the correlations are of clinical significance. the jitter (r = -0.62) displayed a moderate to strong negative correlation with the dsi, implying that participants who scored poorly on this parameter also scored lower than 1.6 on the dsi. in contrast, maximum frequency (r = 0.40) showed a positive relationship with the dsi, indicating that participants with a maximum frequency showed lower than the norm also scored lower than 1.6 on the dsi. table 6: acoustic outcomes and correlations between the stuttering severity instrument, dysphonia severity index and voice handicap index and acoustic variables. spearman’s rank correlation coefficients were calculated for the grbasi scales and ssi, dsi, vhi and participant characteristics (table 7). moderate positive correlation was observed between breathiness and ssi (r = 0.40; p = 0.08). a moderate positive correlation between oesophageal reflux and vhi was noted (r = 0.45; p = 0.05), indicating that the participant with oesophageal reflux scored higher on the vhi. significant differences were observed between home language and the asthenia score (p = 0.024). it appears that sepedi and setswana speakers obtained a weaker rating of 1, compared to english and afrikaans speakers who were rated 0 for asthenia. further significant associations were observed between the strain score and weekly alcohol consumption (p = 0.042), as well as daily consumption of carbonated drinks (p = 0.022). the higher the daily consumption of alcohol and carbonated drinks, the higher is the perceived strain in voicing. no other significant associations between the perceptual evaluations and gender, marital status, medical history and excessive shouting were observed in this study. table 7: correlations between the stuttering severity instrument, dysphonia severity index and voice handicap index and grade of hoarseness, roughness, breathiness, asthenia, strain and instability scale. ethical considerations ethical clearance was obtained from the research ethics committee, department speech language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, prior to data collection, with ethics clearance number: 2016/13024753/04498063/13042964. once written informed consent was provided, the comprehensive voice and speech assessment protocol was conducted. discussion the severity of ds in the current study ranged between very mild to moderate. supporting the outcomes of the current study, a previous study reported a mean stuttering severity in the mild to moderate range (blumgart, craig & tran, 2010). the perceptual analysis of voice has shown moderately positive correlations between breathiness and ssi (p = 0.08). during the reading activity and spontaneous speech sample, 25% of the participants displayed a breathy voice quality. a breathy voice is characterised by air loss through loosely or hypo-adducted vocal folds (ferrand, 2012). breathiness is often accompanied by low vocal intensity and a lower than optimum fundamental frequency level (ferrand, 2012). speech-language therapists often recommend the use of the easy voice onset technique to pws. if pws utilise this technique, they will not adduct their vocal folds as forcefully; therefore, an increased breathy voice quality may be audible (adler, hirsch & mordaunt, 2006). interestingly, the acoustic analysis of voice rendered significantly different mean values for jitter (p ≤ 0.001), maximum frequency (p ≤ 0.001), minimum intensity (p ≤ 0.001), mpt (p = 0.001) and the dsi (p ≤ 0.001) when compared to the norms. the study also indicated that a higher ssi score resulted in a higher dsi score (r = 0.41, p = 0.08). most participants were not able to sustain voicing for 20 s or more. mpt is often reduced in individuals with dysphonia (ferrand, 2012). the reduced mpt may emphasise the fact that the pws may struggle with prolongations of sounds and present with more dysphonic voices (bogaardt, speyer & zumach, 2008). reduced mpt also indicates decreased efficiency of the respiratory mechanism during phonation, causing inefficient use of air in pws (bogaardt et al., 2008). an interesting and unexpected finding was the significant difference between the norms and maximum frequency and minimum intensity and this should be explored in future research. poor jitter values were associated with a poorer dsi and higher vhi score. the jitter may be affected mainly because of lack of control of vocal fold vibration, in the moments of stuttering which may result in the presence of noise at emission and breathiness of the voice (amaro, schreiber & wertzner, 2005). participants who scored higher in the vhi also had a higher asthenia rating in the grbasi scale. asthenia refers to the degree of weakness of the voice. the weakness of the voice can be caused by an overuse of voice or using the voice ineffectively (ferrand, 2012). the pws may change the way they use their voice to compensate for the emotions, anxiety and stuttering symptoms experienced (guitar, 2014). the demographic variances have shown some significant differences between home language and the asthenia score (p = 0.024). it appears that sepedi and setswana speakers obtained a weaker rating of 1, compared to english and afrikaans speakers who were rated 0 for asthenia. a study by boyer and zsiga (2013) has indicated that depending on the geographical area, many of the setswana speakers who participated in their study reported to have post-nasal devoicing. the devoicing of some sounds in the african languages may have contributed to the weaker asthenia ratings. devoicing of sounds might also be interpreted as a weak voice (pinho, jesus & barney, 2009). a noteworthy association between a higher daily consumption of alcohol (p = 0.042) and carbonated drinks (p = 0.022) and higher perceived strain in voicing, was found. the use of excessive alcohol causes dehydration of the vocal folds and may also lead to irritation of the mucous membranes that line the throat (ferrand, 2012). a study by baek and bae (2013a, 2013b) also stated that the use of alcohol can reduce the flexibility and elasticity of the mucosa tissue of the vocal cords. as a result incomplete opening or closure of the glottis reduces clarity of the speaker’s pronunciation because of the air leak when speaking after drinking (geumran & muyungjin, 2013). this study is one of few studies that investigated the vocal characteristics of pws. this explorative study was conducted to identify whether an experimental study is warranted. it is therefore recommended that future research should use an experimental research design on a larger sample size, including a control group that are age and gender matched. further research should also be conducted in the paediatric population as a means to prevent voice disorders in pws by creating awareness and establishing good vocal habits. conclusion a few studies have explored voice problems in pws, and the characteristics of voices of pws are relatively unknown. the who’s icf proposes that it is important to understand the full burden of a health condition. because of limited research in this area and the importance and extent to which stuttering affects a person, the aim was to describe the acoustic and perceptual parameters of voice quality in adults with ds. results obtained from this study indicated that a higher ssi score resulted in a poorer dsi score. it has been found in the current study that the majority of pws presented with dysphonia. further investigation is warranted to establish prevention and intervention of voice disorders in pws. acknowledgements competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions a.b. performed the collection of data and organisation of data collection venues and equipment and played a lead role in data analysis and writing of the article. e.r. performed the collection of data, organisation of data collection venues and equipment and writing of the article. j.l. provided guidance on data collection, data analysis and writing of the article, organisation of data collection venues and equipment. s.a. provided guidance on data collection and writing of the article, organisation of data collection venues and equipment. references adler, r.k., hirsch, s., & mordaunt, m. (2006). voice and communication therapy for the transgender/transsexual client: a comprehensive clinical guide. san diego, ca: plural publishing inc. amaro, l., schreiber, s., & wertzner, h.f. (2005). analysis of fundamental frequency, jitter, shimmer and vocal intensity in children with phonological disorders. brazilian journal of otorhinolarygology, 71(5), 582–588. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1808-8694(15)31261-1 baek, g., & bae, m.j. (2013a). a study on drinking judgment algorithm using speech parameter. international conference on information science and technology, astl 23, 71–73). baek, g., & bae, m. (2013b). a study on voice sobriety test algorithm in a time-frequency domain. international journal of multimedia and ubiquitous engineering, 8(5), 395–402. https://doi.org/10.14257/ijmue.2013.8.5.39 banjara, h., mungutwar, v., singh, d., & gupta, a. (2014). objective and subjective evaluation of larynx in smokers and non-smokers: a comparative study. indian journal of otolaryngology head neck surgery, 66, 99–109. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12070-011-0342-3 blood, g., blood, i., maloney, k., meyer, c., & qualls, c. (2007). anxiety levels in adolescents who stutter. journal of communication disorders, 40(6), 452–469. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2006.10.005 blumgart, e., craig, a., & tran, y. (2010). social anxiety disorders in adults who stutter. depression and anxiety, 27, 687–692. https://doi.org/10.1002/da.20657 bogaardt, h.c.a., speyer, r., & zumach, a. (2008). maximum phonation time: variability and reliability. journal of voice, 24(3), 281–284. boyer, o., & zsiga, e. 2013. phonological devoicing and phonetic voicing in setswana. selected proceedings of the annual conference on african linguistics (acal) 43, 82–89. cooper, m. (1979). the voice problems of stutterers: a practical approach from clinical experience. journal of fluency disorders, 4(2), 141–148. https://doi.org/10.1016/0094-730x(79)90013-5 eckel, f., & boone, d., (1981). the s/z ratio as an indicator of laryngeal pathology. journal of fluency disorders, 46(2), 147–149. https://doi.org/10.1044/jshd.4602.147 erickson, s., & block, s. (2013). the social and communication impact of stuttering on adolescents and their families. journal of fluency disorders, 38(4), 311–324. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jfludis.2013.09.003 fairbanks, g. (1960). voice and articulation drillbook. (2nd edn., pp. 124–139). new york: harper & row. ferrand, c.t. (2012). voice disorders: scope of theory and practice. boston, ma: pearson. frühholz, s., trost, w., & grandjean, d. (2014). the role of the medial temporal limbic system in processing emotions in voice and music. progress in neurobiology, 123, 1–17. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.pneurobio.2014.09.003 guitar, b. (2014). stuttering: an integrated approach to its nature and treatment. (4th edn.) baltimore, md: wolters kluwer. ingham, r.g., fox, p.t., ingham, j.c., xiong, j., zamarripa, f., hardies, j.l., et al. (2004). brain correlates of stuttering and syllable production: gender comparison and replication. journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 47, 321–341. https://doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2004/026 kaypentax. (2008). the software instruction manual of the multi-dimensional voice program (mdvp) model 5105. lincoln park, nj: kaypentax. maertens, k., & de jong, f.i. (2007). the voice handicap index as a tool for assessment of the biopsychosocial impact of voice problems. b-ent, 3(3), 7. maslan, j., leng, x., rees, c., blalock, d., & butler, s.g. (2011). maximum phonation time in healthy older adults. journal of voice, 25, 709–713. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2010.10.002 mukaka, m.m. (2012). a guide to appropriate use of correlation coefficient in medical research. malawi medical journal, 24, 69–71. pinho, c.m., jesus, l.m., & barney, a. (2009). aerodynamics of fricative production in european portuguese. in interspeech (pp. 472–475). pullin, g., & cook, a. (2013). the value of visualising tone of voice. logopedics phoniatrics vocology, 38, 105–114. https://doi.org/10.3109/14015439.2013.809144 riley, g. (2009). the stuttering severity instrument for adults and children (ssi-4). (4th edn.). austin, tx: pro-ed. salihović, n., junuzović-žunić, l., ibrahimagić, a., & beganović, l. (2009). characteristics of voice in stuttering children. acta medica saliniana, 38(2), 67–75. sidtis, d., & kreiman, j. (2011). in the beginning was the familiar voice: personally familiar voices in the evolutionary and contemporary biology of communication. integrative psychological & behavioural science, 46, 146–159. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12124-011-9177-4 van wyk, l., cloete, m., hattingh, d., van der linde, j., & geertsema, s. (2016). the effect of hydration on the voice quality of future professional vocal performers. journal of voice, 31(1), 111.e29–111-36. http://doi.org/10.1016/j.jvoice.2016.01.002 watkins, k.e., smith, s.m., davis, s., & howell, p. (2008). structural and functional abnormalities of the motor system in developmental stuttering. brain, 131, 50–59. https://doi.org/10.1093/brain/awm241 wuyts, f.l., de bodt, m.s., molenberghs, g., remacle, m., heylen, l., millet, b., et al. (2000). the dysphonia severity index: an objective measure of vocal quality based on a multiparameter approach. journal of speech, language and hearing research, 43, 796–780. https://doi.org/10.1044/jslhr.4303.796 xue, s., & deliyski, d. (2001). effects of ageing on selected acoustic voice parameters: preliminary normative data and educational implications. educational gerontology, 27, 159–168. https://doi.org/10.1080/03601270151075561 yamauchi, e.j., imaizumi, s., maruyama, h., & haji, t. (2010). perceptual evaluation of pathological voice quality: a comparative analysis between the rasati and grbasi scales. logopedics phoniatrics vocology, 35, 121–128. https://doi.org/10.3109/14015430903334269 normal acoustic reflex amplitude growth and the influence of cochlear hearing loss juliette womersley b.sc(log) (cape town) lucille dickens b(log) (pretoria) department of logopaedics, university of cape town abstract the nature of acoustic reflex amplitude (ara) growth at ikhz and 2khz was investigated in normal hearing and cochlear disordered subjects subdivided into meniere's disease and heterogeneous pathology groups. statistical and graphical analyses revealed significant inter-group variation in ara growth rate. the normal and meniere's groups behaved similarly, while the heterogeneous group demonstrated a faster ara growth rate. the differential sensitivity of various measurement methods was examined. explanations were put forward to account for variability in ara data amongst cochlear disordered subjects. it was concluded that the clinical sensitivity of ara measurement was questionable. opsomming die toename van die akoestiese refleks amplitude (ara) by ikhz en 2khz is in normaalhorendes en proejpersone met kogliere letsels ondersoek. laasgenoemde is in meniere se siekte en groepe met 'n heterogene patologie onderverdeel. 'n betekenisvolle variasie tussen groepe m. b. t. die ara-groeitempo is d. m. v. statistiese en graflese analises bevind. ooreenkomste tussen die normale en die meniere se groepe is aangetref terwyl daar 'n toename in die ara-groeitempo van die heterogene groep was. verder is die sensitiwiteitsverskil van verskeie metingsmetodes ondersoek. ten einde die veranderlikheid in ara-data~tussen proefpersone met kogliere letsels te verantwoord, is verskeie verklarings gebied. die gevolgtrekking is gemaak dat die kliniese sensitiwiteit van die ara-meting bevraagteken is. the role of acoustic reflex measurement in determining the nature of sensori-neural hearing loss has been well established. traditionally the acoustic reflex threshold test, a static measure, and the reflex decay test, a dynamic temporal measure, are employed for this purpose (jerger, 1975). measurement of other dynamic properties such as amplitude and latency has not yet achieved the same clinical status. borg (1976) however, stated that such properties may contribute significantly to clinical diagnosis in impedance audiometry if they are proven to be pathology sensitive. acoustic reflex amplitude (ara) is defined as the change in acoustic impedance between quiescent and reflexive states. ara is intensity dependent: for pure tone stimulation it has a dynamic range of 20-30 db, representing the intensity range over which the reflex shows an amplitude growth (wilson and mcbride, 1978). the nature of ara growth in normal subjects has been variably reported in the literature. uliel (1980) and clemis and sarno (1980) reported a linear function while several other investigators have reported a curvilinear function (dallos, 1964; sprague, wiley and block, 1981; wilson and mcbride, 1978). inter-study variability in the description of normal ara growth could have resulted from a nonstandardised basis of amplitude measurement; sensation level (sl) versus hearing level (hl) measurement methods. sprague et al. (1981) employed both sl and hl in their data analysis and demonstrated an ara configuration difference accordingly. this lack of agreement between researchers is also evident in the literature on ara in the cochlear hearing loss population. uliel (1980) investigated an ascending-descending ara function in normal and cochlear disordered subjects, by hl measurement. the growth pattern characteristic of cochlear disordered subjects without concomitant loudness recruitment was similar to that of the normal group. in contrast, cochlear disordered subjects with concomitant loudness recruitment demonstrated a faster than normal © sasha 1985 amplitude growth. an increased growth rate was also repeatedly observed by clemis and sarno (1980) in meniere's disease subjects, at ikhz and 2khz. in contrast with these findings, petersen and liden (1972) and beedie and harford (1973) reported a slower growth rate in pathological ears of variable cochlear etiology, than in normal ears. both uliel (1980) and beedle and harford (1973) specifically investigated ears with loudness recruitment and thus the discrepancy in their respective findings is particularly notable. a further confusion is that jerger and hayes (1983) reported an abnormally slow growth rate to be characteristic of a retrocochlear group. these discrepancies indicate that the influence of cochlear pathology on ara has not been unequivocally established. furthermore, since uliel (1980) did not find an abnormal growth rate to be characteristic of alljcochlear disordered subjects, it is logical to suspect that the influence of cochlear disorder on ara might vary as a function of pathology. the need for further research on ara in the cochlear population is clear. however, results cannot be classified validly as "faster" or "slower" than normal, until "normal" growth has been unequivocally defined. further investigation in to the shape of the ara growth function in normal subjects is therefore also necessary. methodology aims the amplitude growth of the acoustic reflex was investigated at ikhz and 2khz, in normal hearing and cochlear disordered subjects with the aim of 1. describing the configuration of the ara growth function in normal hearing subjects for various measurement methods, namely: hl, sl, δ hl, and δ sl; the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) normal acoustic reflex amplitude growth and the influence of cochlear hearing loss 59 2. investigating the influence of cochlear dysfunction on the ara growth function. specifically, to investigate ara in a group of meniere's disease subjects, and a group of subjects with variable cochlear etiology, excluding meniere's disease; 3. examining the variable sensitivity of different measurement methods in distinguishing between the normal and pathological subject groups. subjects the subject sample in this investigation comprised three groups: a control group (a) of 16 normal hearing subjects (mean age 24 years; 38 test ears), an experimental group (b) of six subjects with meniere's disease (mean age 38 years; six test ears), and a further experimental group (c) of four subjects with variable cochlear etiology, excluding meniere's disease, (mean age 40 years, six test ears). this age limit of 20-50 years was imposed since ara data has been shown to be relatively stable across this age range (osterhammel and osterhammel, 1979). both sexes were represented. subjects were selected on the basis of case-history findings and audiologic results obtained by the author from a battery comprising pure tone air and bone conduction audiometry, impedance audiometry, the metz recruitment and rosenburg tone decay tests. table 1 summarises the specific criteria according to which subjects were differentiated into groups a, β and c. equipment a madsen electro-acoustic impedance audiometer (model z073a) which delivered a 220 hz probe tone was used. this was calibrated according to standards set out in iec publication 318. a hewlett packard moseley x-y plotter (model 7035a) was connected to the impedance meter. the x-axis was activated by depression of the pure tone stimulus interrupter switch on the impedance meter, resulting in a time-locked 2 second excursion. presentation of the 250 msec stimulus followed automatically but not immediately, allowing a baseline to be plotted before reflex elicitation. the impedance meter was set at sensitivity 2 and the range of the y-axis was calibrated such that 0.05 cc = 0.04 mv = 1 mm on the chart paper. an acoustic reflex was recorded as a relative deflection from the baseline, representing an increase in input impedance (refer to figure 1). experimental procedure tympanometry was performed on each test ear prior to experimental testing in order to determine the point of maximum compliance. the acoustic reflex testing was conducted by the contralateral stimulus mode at lkhz and 2khz. the starting point for stimulus intensity was subject specific: testing began at reflex threshold and table 1 summary of specific criteria for subject selection and grouping group a group β group c detailed case-history negative history of hearing difficulty, ear pathology, otologic surgery, noise exposure and ototoxic drug intake. positive history of cochlear sensori neural hearing loss, negative history of middle ear pathology and surgery, noise exposure, acoustic trauma, and ototoxic drug intake. positive history of meniere's disease and associated symptomatology, confirmed by an e.n.t. specialist. positive history of cochlear sensori-neural hearing loss, negative history of middle ear pathology and otologic surgery and meniere's disease. hearing thresholds no poorer than 25 db. no air-bone gap. between 25-75 db at speech frequencies. no air-bone gap. between 25-75 db at speech frequencies. no air-bone gap. tympanometry type-a tympanogram to rule out the presence of middle ear disorders. type-a tympanogram to rule out the presence of middle ear disorders. type-a tympanogram to rule out the presence of middle ear disorders. static compliance 0.55cc-1.5cc. ; 0.5cc-1.55cc. 0.5cc-1.5cc. reflex thresholds between 70-100 db at all frequencies. between 70-100 db at lkhz and 2khz, contralateral^. between 70-100 db at lkhz and 2khz, contralaterally. supra-threshold dynamic range minimum of 25 dbhl to allow for sufficient ara growth. minimum of 25 dbhl to allow for sufficient ara growth. minimum of 25 dbhl to allow for sufficient ara growth. metz test negative. positive: arsl < 60 db. arsl < 60 db. tone decay test — negative. negative. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 juliette womersley and lucille dickens ν ό d "5. ε, < φ cc u> 3 ο o < mv 0.04 mv = 1 mm 2 sec. excursion reflex amplitude figure 1 a sample reflex response at 95 dbhl, illustrating the configuration of reflex amplitude, a = interrupter switch depression: the distance between a and b represents the baseline before reflex elecitation. proceeded in 5 db increments up to 125 dbhl. in this manner a series of graphical figures (refer to figure 1) were obtained representing reflex amplitude at consecutive stimulus levels, for each test ear at both frequencies. analysis of results the graphical figures represented the raw data. the amplitude of each reflex was established by calculating the millimetre measurement from the lowest point of the positive deflection to the highest point representing the maximal increase in input impedance. millimetre measurements were converted to mv's according to the scale — 1 mm = 0.04 mv. mv scores used in data analysis were restricted to those at hearing levels (hl) and sensation levels (sl) at which a reflex response was common to all test ears. data were also tabulated separately for those normal subjects who demonstrated the broadest dynamic range of amplitude growth, thus forming a subgroup of the normal sample. the following analyses were performed: 1. graphical analysis mean ara functions for the three subject groups were graphically displayed with the aim of examining the influence of subject group and measurement method variables on the configuration of these functions. data for the subgroup of normal subjects was graphically displayed so as to examine the effect of dynamic range on function configuration. 2 . slope index measurement slope index values were calculated for each graphical figure, based either on the whole hl or sl stimulus range (100-125 dbhl or 0-25 dbsl) or based only on the final two hl or sl increments (120-125 dbhl or 20-25 dbsl); thus observed differences in overall or 'tail-end' function configurations could be quantified. ' 3 . statistical analysis the two-way analysis of variance statistic, with repeated measures on β (2-anova-rb), was used to establish whether a significant interaction existed between subject group (variable a) and stimulus level (variable b). the simple main effects (sme) and tukey's honestly significant difference (hsd) statistics were used to quantify further the variation in ara between subject groups at specific stimulus levels. all hl and sl data at lkhz and 2khz were converted to δ hl and δ sl data, by computing the mv difference between scores at consecutive stimulus levels. this data was analysed graphically with the aim of examining the influence of δ hl and δ sl measurement methods on inter-group trends. results and discussion normals the graphical representation of data from 38 test ears at lkhz and 2khz, over a 100-125 dbhl range and a 0-25 dbsl range, revealed that the nature of ara growth was neither mathematically linear or curvilinear (refer to figure 2). this finding contrasts with the relevant literature which has variably reported ara configurations in normal subject groups to be exactly linear (uliel, 1980; clemis and sarno, 1980) or curvilinear (dallos, 1964; sprague et al., 1981; wilson and mcbride, 1978). it is possible that this discrepancy between present and other research findings is a function of the small sample size and considerable variability in function configurations, that characterised this study. mv ν •ό q. ε < x φ φ cc ο ο < key χ = hl,1khz ο = sl,1khz • = hl,2khz δ = sl,2khz _ ι _ 100 105 10 15 db sl 110 115 db hl 20 25 120 125 figure 2 ara functions for normal subjects at lkhz and 2 khz for hl and sl data / the differential effect of measurement method on ara configuration was visually evident at 2khz but not at lkhz (refer figure 2). there was no difference between the hl-sl configurations at lkhz, whereas at 2khz an asymptotic function was characteristic of the sl but not the hl function. ^ the obtained 'tail-end' slope index values were as follows: hl 0.3 mv (lkhz) sl 0.3 mv (lkhz) hl 0.5 mv (2khz) / s l o.'l mv (2khz) these values substantiated the graphical findings: while there was no difference between the hl and sl values at lkhz, the sl index the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) normal acoustic reflex amplitude growth and the influence of cochlear hearing loss 61 mv ν ό "5. ε < x φ φ cc ο ΰ ) 3 ο ο < 30 dbsl 100-125 dbhl ° key: χ = 1 khz ο = 2κηζ _l _l _ l _l _1_ _ l 85 95 105 db hl 115 125 figure 3 a complete ara function for a sample of 10 normal subjects, with super imposed hl and sl ranges, at ikhz and 2khz at 2khz was reduced relative to the hl value, indicating an asymptotic function. this hl-sl configuration difference is in keeping with the findings of sprague et al (1981), and requires an explanation particularly in view of it's frequency selectively in this study. figure 3 illustrates an ara function for the subgroup of normal subjects who demonstrated a broad dynamic range (85-125 dbhl) at ikhz and 2khz. the superimposition of hl and sl ranges on the graphical figures reveals that the differences'in function configuration description (such as asymptotic versus non-asymptotic) can result from anal y s e s ^ different portions of a wide intensity range. this possibly explains discrepancy in descriptions reported in the literature: linear versus curvilinear. figure 3 illustrates further that the absence of an asymptotic sl function at ikhz was simply a function of the breadth of dynamic range underi investigation, namely 25 dbsl; an asymptotic function would have resulted had a 30 dbsl range been investigated. considerable variability was found in the rate and pattern of ara growth amongst normal subjects. this inter-subject variability was pronounced for hl measurement and relatively reduced for sl measurement, in keeping with the findings of petersen and linden (1972). a broad range of normal variability might obscure the clinical identification of mild or even moderate pathological deviance, and therefore it is implied that sl measurement offers a better prognosis for clinical sensitivity in ara testing, than hl measurement, by reducing this range. inter-group comparisons graphical representation of the function configurations for groups a, b, and c, for sl data at ikhz and 2khz revealed a marked trend; group c demonstrated a visually steeper ara function (faster ara growth rate), while groups a and β were similar regardless of stimulus parameters (refer to figures 4 and 5). this trend was also characteristic of hl data, at 1 and 2khz, although less pronounced. whole slope index values quantified and confirmed these findings: the highest values were consistently obtained for group c, indicating a faster ara growth rate in this group than in either groups a or β (refer to table 2). mv φ •ό α . ε < φ cc 3 ο ϋ < key χ = group α ο = group β δ = group c 15 20 25 0 5 10 db sl figure 4 ara functions at ikhz, for sl data, showing inter-group growth rate trends mv 5 key x = group a ο = group β δ = group c φ • σ 3 t 3 ε < χ φ φ cc • i 2 ο ο < 1 ' 5 10 15 20 25 db sl figure 5 ara functions at 2khz, for sl data, showing inter-group growth rate trends die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 juliette womersley and lucille dickens table 2 whole-slope index mv values for groups a, β and c group hl, 1 khz sl, 1 khz a 0,4 0,4 β 0,3 0,5 c 0,6 0,8 hl, 2 khz sl, 2 khz a 0,3 0,4 β 0,3 0,5 c 0,8 0,8 the 2-anova-rb statistic for hl and sl measurement at ikhz and 2khz, yielded significant f-ratio values (p<0.01, and ρ <0.05 for sl data at 2khz). the sme statistic indicated that significant variation within all subjects (variable a collapsed), existed at specific stimulus levels for ikhz and 2khz, these levels being more numerous for sl than hl data, and for 1 khz and 2 khz data. these findings suggest that the 1 khz stimulus and the sl measurement method were more sensitive to inter-subject variation, and therefore have greater potential clinical value. the tukey's t-test results quantified inter-group variability at the specific stimulus levels identified by the sme statistic. the similarity between groups a and β suggested by graphical and slope index analyses was confirmed: tukey's t values in the group a group β comparison never reached statistical significance. in contrast statistical significance was revealed in the group c — group a, and group c — group β comparisons, but only at specific stimulus levels. these statistical results suggest that the label of a 'faster than normal ara growth rate' in group c, is only valid at specific stimulus levels. the discussion that follows however, is based on overall trends of inter-group comparisons, since the implications arising from statistical analyses on a small subject sample may be misleading. the finding of a faster than normal ara growth rate in the heterogeneous pathology group (c) agrees with the research of clemis and sarno (1980) who found this to be characteristic of cochlear disordered subjects, but is contrary to the findings of petersen and liden (1972) and beedle and harford (1973) who found a slower growth rate in cochlear disordered subjects. the similarity in growth rate between groups a and β was unexpected since clemis and sarno (1980) repeatedly observed a faster than normal ara growth rate in meniere's disease subjects, as did sprague et al. (1981) in a single case of meniere's disease. the dissimilarity in ara growth rate between groups β and c was surprising in view of their common lineage both groups belonged to the cochlear population. this dissimilarity raises two issues for •discussion: firstly, the variability in ara data within the cochlear population requires an explanation, and secondly, the interpretation or meaning of a faster than normal ara growth rate must be questioned. clearly, a faster than normal ara growth rate cannot be interpreted as being indicative of cochlear pathology since subjects in the population have also yielded a slower than normal ara growth rate (petersen and liden, 1972) and a normal growth rate as shown in this study. the term 'recruitment' has been used by uliel (1980) and clemis and sarno (1980) in association with a faster than normal ara growth rate. the use of this term in this context does not agree with other research. beedle and harford (1973) specifically investigated cochlear subjects with concomitant loudness recruitment and found a slower ara growth rate. furthermore, in this study all experimental subjects demonstrated loudness recruitment on the metz test, however only those subjects in group c yielded the faster ara growth rate. an alternative interpretation of intra-cochlear variability and a faster ara growth rate, is based on the relationship between cochlear pathology and the neurological basis of the ara response system. evans (1982) states that damage to a cochlear fibre causes a broadening of its frequency threshold curve and a consequent increase in the rate at which adjacent cochlear fibres become activated as a function of increasing stimulus intensity. if this is an acceptable explanation of a faster ara growth rate, then it is implied that cochlear fibre functioning in the meniere's disease group was normal, since this group yielded an ara growth rate mapping that of normal subjects. no definitive statements can be made regarding the differential effect of cochlear pathology on hair cell morphology in groups β and c, however, the variability in state and stage of disease that is characteristic of meniere's disease patients (brackman, selters and don, 1982) may be of significance when contrasted with the profile of subjects in group c, all of whom reported a minimum 10 year history of bilateral hearing loss with no fluctuating symptomatology. it would be of interest in future research to specifically compare a group of subjects with known hair cell damage, with a group of 'early' stage meniere's disease patients who had shown symptom-reversability on glycerol testing (thus suggesting no permanent hair cell damage). audiogram configuration is another variable which may have contributed to the dissimilarity between groups β and c. five of the six meniere's subjects showed predominantly low frequency hearing loss whereas this was not characteristic of subjects in group c. future research may reveal that lower test frequencies (below ikhz) are more sensitive to meniere's disease and therefore elicit a faster ara growth rate in these subjects. mv 1.25 1.00 <1) t3 3 ε 0.75 < x α> α> a 0.50 ο <0 ο ο < 0.25 100-105 110-115 120-125 db δ hl χ ' figure 6 ara functions for δ hl data at 1 khz, illustrating the ascending-descending configuration similarity between groups β and c, relative to group a the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) normal acoustic reflex amplitude growth and the influence of cochlear hearing loss 63 these explanations put forward to account for intra-cochlear variability are tentative owing to the use of small sample sizes, and particularly in view of the degree of normal variability in ara data in this study. graphical analyses of δ hl and δ sl data suggested that groups β and c behaved similarly, in contrast with the trend revealed by hl and sl data (refer figure 6). this similarity manifested itself in an ascending-descending configuration which was riot characteristic of group a. the meaning of this configuration is not certain, however the implication is that δ hl and δ sl measurement methods were more successful than all other methods in distinguishing between normal and cochlear subjects per se. in view of this implication, it is possible that the intra-cochlear variability in this study was a deceptive consequence of measurement method and not a real phenomenon. this does not alter the intra-cochlear variability found within uliel's (1980) study and between other studies (clemis and sarno, 1980; petersen and liden, 1972; beedle and harford, 1973), which remains worthy of note and is as yet not successfully accounted for. conclusions the configuration of ara growth functions obtained for normal hearing subjects were shown to be influenced by the method of ara measurement, namely hl versus sl measurement. this variable determined which area of dynamic range of amplitude growth was analysed, and consequently determined the asymptotic versus nonasymptotic configuration difference. the same variable may also account for discrepancies in ara configuration description in the literature. if this hypothesis is correct then the implication is that a standardised methodological basis for ara is required, because without reliable normative data the relative influence of hearing impairment cannot be successfully determined. on the basis of hl and sl measurement methods, the meniere's disease group yielded an ara growth rate similar to that of normal subjects, while the heterogeneous pathology group showed a faster than normal ara growth rate. this intra-cochlear variability has also been found in other studies and suggests that ara measurement has a poor prognosis for-clinical sensitivity. this is implied since'faster than normal ara growth rate (positive result) might indicate cochlear disorder but a negative test result would not contraindicate cochlear disorder. δ hl and δ sl measurement methods distinguished, to a certain extent, between the normal and cochlear disordered subjects per se, and therefore offer a better prognosis for test sensitivity than either the hl or sl methods. it could be the purpose of future research, therefore, to examine and compare these and other alternative measurement methods with the aim of improving clinical sensitivity to the degree that would be necessary if ara measurement was to be included in the impedance audiometry test battery. references beedle, r.k., and harford, e.r. comparison of the acoustic reflex and loudness growth in normal and pathological ears. j. speech hear. res., 16, 271-281, 1973. borg, e. dynamic characteristics of the intra-aural muscle reflex. in acoustic impedance and admittance — the measurement of middle ear function. feldman, α., wilber, l. (eds.). the williams & wilkins co., baltimore, u.s.a., 1976. brackman, d.e., selter, w.a., and don, m. auditory evoked responses. in otolaryngology 1: otology, gibb, a.g. & smith, h. (eds.) butterworth & co., london, 1982. clemis, m.d., and sarno, c.n. the acoustic reflex latency test: clinical application. laryngoscope, 90, 601-610, 1980. dallos, p. dynamics of the acoustic reflex: phenomenological aspects. j. acous. soc. am., 36, 2175, 1964. evans, e.f. recent advances in cochlear physiology. in otolaryngology 1: otology. gibb, a.g., & smith, m. (eds.) butterworth & co., london, 1982. jerger, j. handbook of clinical impedance, chap. 7. american electro medics corp., new york, 1975. jerger, j., and hayes, d. latency of the acoustic reflex in eighth nerve tumour. arch. otolaryngol., 109, 1-5, 1983. osterhammel, p., and osterhammel, d. age and sex variations for the normal stapedial reflex thesholds and tympanometric compliance values. scand. audio., 8, 153-158, 1979. petersen, j. and liden, g. some static characteristics of the stapedial muscle reflex. audiology, 11, 97, 1972. sprague, b.h., wiley, t.l., and block., m.g. dynamics of acoustic reflex growth. audiology, 20, 15-40, 1981. uliel, s. acoustic reflex measurements and the loudness function in sensori-neural hearing loss. s.a.j. commun. dis., 27, 58-77, 1980. wilson, r.h. and mcbride, l.m. threshold and growth of the acoustic reflex. j. acous. soc. am., 63, 147-154, 1978. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) in close cooperation with the electro-acoustic department of siemens, one of the world's leading electrical and electronic engineering companies, r e p u b l i c 2 11h e a r i n g a l d consultants (pty) ltd »»)))) 187 jan smuts avenue lower rosebank offers hearing aids (opposite mupps) a n ( j school teaching tel.: 442-8691 . ^ equipment of the highest quality. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the effects of mandibular osteotomy on articulation and resonance delsa geffen m.a.(log.) (pretoria) psychological and guidance service, transvaal education department, johannesburg north. summary preand post-operative speech samples were studied in nine adult cases who received mandibular osteotomy. lateral cephalograms were taken during sustained production of selected sounds and trained listeners judged recordings. in most cases there was an improvement in the general quality of the speech. considering thai the functional relationships between the speech organs had altered, it would appear that some form of adaptation by the speaker had in fact taken place. opsomming preen postoperatiewe spraakvoorbeelde is bestudeer van nege volwasse gevalle wat mandibulere osteotomie ondergaan het. laterale kefalogramme is geneem tydens die volgehoue produksie van sekere klanke en opgeleide luisteraars het opnames geevalueer. in die meeste gevalle het die algemene kwaliteit van die spraak verbeter. gesien dat die funksionele verhoudings tussen die spraakorgane verander het, wil dit voorkom asof adaptasie wel by die spreker plaasgevind het. surgical correction of prognathic and retrognathic conditions of the mandible is being undertaken with increasing frequency throughout the world. mandibular osteotomy, in correcting an abnormal jaw relationship, brings about certain changes affecting the speech organs. there is a modification in the relative positions of the structures of the oral cavity. these include the palate, tongue, teeth and lips, all of which are involved in articulation. an alteration also takes place in the dimensions of the oral cavity, as well as in the size and shape of the orifice, resulting from the change in position of the teeth and lips. the aim of this study was to investigate the effects of. mandibular osteotomy on those speech parameters which are functions of the supralaryngeal cavities, namely articulation and resonance. although many maxillo-facial and oral surgeons have commented on the improvement of speech in their patients after correction of an abnormal jaw relationship,4' 7> 9' " few controlled studies have been published. goodstein et al3 studied five persons with mandibular prognathism, all of whom had deviant articulation. they found no significant change in the articulation of their subjects1 after mandibular osteotomy, but reported that voice quality improved. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mandibular osteotomy and articulation 55 method nine adults requiring mandibular osteotomy for the correction of a malrelationship between the maxilla and the mandible were studied. two patients had received speech therapy during childhood for defective production of /s/, but without success. as research has indicated a direct relationship between the physiological processes of articulation and the acoustic properties of the sound produced,2 both of these aspects were investigated. auditory acoustic investigation a comparison was made between a pre-operative speech sample and a post-operative speech sample in each case. two short passages were selected, one in english and the other in afrikaans, containing all the phonemes of the language concerned. each subject was required to read the passage in his mother tongue. the post-operative speech evaluations were made between three and eleven months after the operation, the average time lapse being five-and-a-half months, thus ensuring complete recovery of the inferior alveolar nerve, recovery of mandibular movement and allowing the patients sufficient time to adjust to the altered structures so that their speech patterns could be considered stable. evaluations of the speech samples were made by five trained listeners. the recordings of each subject were played consecutively to facilitate comparison, but were presented in a random order with regard to preoperative and post-operative speech in order to prevent possible bias in the judgement. the listeners were asked to make comparative intrasubject judgements with regard to the articulation of the different sounds, paying particular attention to the production of /s/ as the accuracy of this sound is so readily disturbed by any dental irregularity.6 the listeners were also asked to consider any additional factors, such as resonance, and projection and quality of the voice. physiological investigation the place of articulation was studied by means of radiography. as cinefluorography was not available, lateral cephalograms were made during the sustained production of selected speech sounds. a narrow strip of ray-tec gauze, approximately 25 mm in length, was placed on the anterior section of the hard palate and another on the anterior part of the tongue, with the radiopaque thread running along the midline of each organ. a thin film of orabase paste was applied to the gauze to ensure adherence to the contours of the palate and tongue during articulation. in the case of english-speaking subjects, the sounds chosen were /θ, s, 1, i/; in the case of afrikaans-speaking subjects /s, 1, i/. in analysing the cephalograms, attention was paid to the salient points regarding the relationships of the organs.'0 the following measurements were taken for the consonants /θ, s, 1/:die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings. vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 delsa geffen a the distance between the upper and lower incisors on a horizontal plane. where a reversed relationship occurred, with the lower incisors anterior to the upper incisors, the distance is expressed as a minus quantity. λ β the distance between the upper and lower incisors on a vertical plane. where the incisal edge of the lower teeth was superior to that of the upper teeth, the distance is expressed as a minus quantity. c the distance, on a horizontal plane, posterior to the cutting edge of the upper anterior incisors, of the narrowest point of the constriction. where this point was doubtful or difficult to determine, the midpoint of the constriction was used.8 for the vowel / i / the horizontal and vertical distances between the upper and lower incisors were noted in the same way. the place of articulation was more difficult to evaluate as, in some cases, the highest point of the tongue was too far posterior to be marked on the cephalogram. to obtain an idea of the configuration and position of the tongue, the width of the air channel was measured (i.e. the distance between the palate and the tongue) at the following set points posterior to the upper anterior incisors: d 10 mm ε 15 mm f 20 mm results measurements obtained in the analysis of the cephalograms are given in table i. important points relating to the cephalograms and the listener judgements will be mentioned in individual cases. a comparison of the articulation of / s / as produced pre-operatively and post-operatively by all subjects, on both the auditory acoustic level and the physiological level, may be found in table ii. the general quality of the speech is also indicated. prior to surgery, case 7 sometimes held the mandible in a forward position when speaking, claiming that she found this more comfortable. an additional cephalogram was taken pre-operatively, viz. of the articulation of / s / with the mandible in the forward position. in cases 1, 8 and 9 the sound / l / was exaggerated when produced in isolation. as it was felt not to be a true reflection of that person's speech, those tracings were not analysed. p r o g n a t h i c m a n d i b l e case 1 cephalograms: the sounds were dentalized pre-operatively. postoperatively, the articulation was modified in accordance with the change in the mandible, and the sounds remained dentalized. listener judgement: both pre-operatively and post-operatively the articulation sounded further forward than normal. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mandibular osteotomy and articulation 57 c ep ha lo m et ri c tr ac in g u. u ο m· οο ο c ep ha lo m et ri c tr ac in g > cq ο < γc ep ha lo m et ri c tr ac in g so un d m ea su re m en ts pr eop er at iv e p os top er at iv e pr eop er at iv e po st -o pe ra ti ve pr eop er at iv e p os top er at iv e pr eop er at iv e po st -o pe ra ti ve pr eop er at iv e po st -o pe ra ti ve pr eop er at iv e m an di bl e fo rw ar d po st -o pe ra ti ve p re -o pe ra ti ve p os top er at iv e pr eop er at iv e po st -o pe ra ti ve d is ta nc e m an di bl e m ov ed ο 00 00 ιη ο ιη i 1 c as e ο μ· ιη γοο ja w re la ti on sh ip pr eop er at iv el y p ro gn at hi c m an di bl e r et ro gn at hi c m an di bl e pr og na th ic m ax ill a fa ci al as ym m et ry i » ε .ο μ ο υ ο od ο ο . ιυ u c < ω _ι cq < η ιλ c ε υ w ε s υ ιλ 3 υ ν) γ-η c 1 1 = η die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 delsa geff e n case 2 c e p h a l o g r a m s : this case was excluded from the cephalometric investigation as he wore a partial denture which was constructed from radiopaque material, with the result that the outlines of the tongue and palate could not be visualized on x-ray. listener judgement: in the pre-operative recording /s/ and / r / were defective and the consonant clusters were indistinct. in the postoperative recording both the accuracy of articulation and the quality of speech were much improved. case 3 cephalograms: pre-operatively the constriction for /s/ was pre-palatal. post-operatively there was a change in tongue position, corresponding to that of the mandibular teeth. judgement of listeners cephalogram of /s/ jaw place of relationship articulageneral articulation adjustment precase tion of quality adjusted in in tongue operatively /s/ /s/ of speech accordance posture with altered jaw position prognathic 1 remained no change yes no mandible defective 2 improved improved — 3 remained improved yes \ n defective 4 remained improved no slight acceptable 5 remained deteriorated no no acceptable 6 remained improved slightly yes defective retrognathic 7 remained deteriorated yes no mandible defective prognathic 8 improved improved yes 'yes maxilla facial 9 remained no change no, but no no asymmetry defective significant anatomical change table ii listener judgement and cephalograms for preand postoperative production of /s/. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 59 m a n d i b u l a r osteotomy and articulation r i.ihgement: in the pre-operative recording the mid-alveolar l i s t c i s s o u n d e d pre-palatal. post-operatively the /s/ segment remained deviant, but the speech was generally "pleasing". k i l o g r a m s · the place of articulation remained the same post% p r a t e i y as pre-operatively, but slight variations occurred e.g. h ^ x ^ ^ o , was good on both recordings. postoperatively the resonance was greatly improved. s i a l o g r a m s : the place of articulation was virtually unchanged postnerativelv in spite of the alteration in the jaw relationship. s e n e r judgement: articulation was good in both recordings, but r e s o n a n c e was poorer post-operatively. case 6 . . cephalograms: both preand post-operatively the constriction for /s/ occurred anterior to the usual position. listener judgement: with regard to articulat.on the /s was el to be deviant in both recordings. the speech was generally better postoneratively, however. of the six patients who underwent surgery to correct a prognathic m a n d i b l e it may be seen that four distorted / s / pre-operatively. postot^erative'ly three of them altered the place of constriction for the sound iifaccordance with the changed mandibular position. in the case of the u b s who produced an acceptable /s/ post-operat.vely, the position of the constriction in relation to the maxillary incisors was maintained. vhe p ace of constriction in relation to the maxilla remained constant in the remaining two cases, who produced / s / correctly both pre ner s and post-operatively. in one case, however, a slight x s t m e m in ton£ie posture occurred, so that a narrower constriction the^ge^eral^quality of speech was maintained or improved in most cases. r e t r o g n a t h i c m a n d i b l e case 7 l^ase / . cenhaloerams· in the pre-operative cephalogram with the mandible held .n a'forward position, and in the post-operative cephalogram there was an extremely small orifice for the escape of air on / s / . l i s e n e r judgement: resonance was found to be best pre-operatively wuh the mandible in the natural position. the /s/ was defect,ve in all " a ^ n l y o n e subject with a r e t r o g n a t h i c mandible. her articulation remained the same but the general quality of her speech deteriorated post-operatively. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 delsa geffen prognathic maxilla case 8 cephalograms: pre-operatively, for the production of /s/, the lower incisors are in contact with the upper alveolar ridge. the tongue humps considerably at the point of constriction and then drops sharply anterior to this, probably to form a channel of sufficient width to conduct the air stream. post-operatively the mandibular incisors are in a lower position and there is thus more space behind the upper incisors and adjacent alveolar ridge. articulation takes place in a more anterior position and the superior surface of the tongue is flatter, with a gentle rise to form the constriction. listener judgement: / s / is deviant pre-operatively, but improved postoperatively. the speech generally and in particular the quality of the voice, is better post-operatively. following the operation in this case, the articulation of / s / was improved by an adjustment in the place of articulation, in addition to modification of the configuration of the tongue. facial asymmetry case 9. .. . cephalograms: the operation resulted in a comparatively small change in the relation of the lower teeth to the upper teeth. the articulation of / s / occurred far forward in the mouth in both cases. listener judgement: the / s / was deviant at times both pre-operatively and post-operatively. there was no significant difference in speech between the two recordings. with no significant anatomical change produced surgically, there was no change in articulation or resonance of this subject's speech. discussion and conclusions post-operatively, the anatomical structures are such that the person may have the potential for normal speech. the acoustic impression of his speech, however, will be determined by the complexity of muscular movements which he makes in relation to his oral morphology.1 in the limited number of subjects studied, certain effects on articulation and resonance were observed. in most cases there was an improvement in the general quality of the speech, which may be attributed to the creation of a more favourable oral environment for optimal resonance. this substantiates the finding that an increase of space between the dorsum of the tongue and the palate brings about a corresponding increase in the carrying power of the voice.5 pre-operatively the relationship between the jaws is such that the orifice of the mouth is basically on a horizontal plane. in the postoperative state, there is a greater space between the maxillary and mandibular teeth in the vertical dimension, through which speech may be emitted. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mandibular osteotomy and articulation 61 in the subject with the retrognathic mandible the general quality of speech deteriorated post-operatively. surgery included a myotomy .of the anterior digastricus and the geniohyoid muscles, and the possibility of the deterioration of speech quality being related to a muscular change in the posture or support of the tongue warrants further investigation. with regard to articulation, when the place of production of the sound was adjusted in accordance with the new position of the teeth and jaw, no change occurred in the acoustic properties. such a change did occur, however, when the place of production was adjusted and, in addition, the posture of the tongue was modified. when the place of production remained the same, the acoustic properties of the sound changed. in view of the alteration of the jaw relationship, the maintenance of the place of articulation implies a marked change in the functional relationship between the speech organs. adjustments made by the speaker in adapting to the alteration in the oral morphology will either facilitate or restrict any changes perceived in his speech. the effects of oral surgery on the speech of the individual can therefore not be predetermined, and any generalizations should be made with caution. certain implications arise with regard to speech therapy for articulatory defects in persons with an abnormal jaw relationship. while defective articulation might be related to such an anomaly, surgical correction of the malrelationship will not necessarily result in normal articulation. this may be accounted for by the habitual patterns of muscular movements, maintained by the characteristic functioning of the various feedback systems of the individual. in two cases, where speechtherapy was attempted prior to surgery,, it was not successful. although this aspect was not included in the present investigation, it is suggested that the better functional relationship between the structures of the oral cavity post-operatively, may facilitate treatment. a ckno wledgements the writer wishes to thank prof. p.c. snijman, dean of the faculty of dentistry, university of pretoria, for his kind permission to utilize the facilities and equipment of the oral and dental hospital. appreciation is also extended to prof. j.g. duvenage, head of the department of maxillo-facial and oral surgery and to prof. j.e. seelige'r, head of the department of radiology, university of pretoria for their co-operation and assistance. references 1. bloomer, h.h. (1971): speech defects associated with dental malocclusions and related abnormalities. in handbook of speech pathology and audiology, travis, l.e. (ed.). appleton-centurycrofts, new york. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 delsa geffen 2. broad, d.j. and peterson, g.e. (1971): the acoustics of speech. in handbook of speech pathology and audio logy, travis, l.e. (ed.). appleton-century-crofts, new york. 3. goodstein, d.b., cooper, d. and wallace, l. (1974): the effect on speech of surgery for correction of mandibular prognathism. oral surg., 37, 846-849. 4. hogeman, κ. e. (1951): surgical-orthopaedic correction of mandibular protrusion. acta chir. scand. suppl. 159. 5. lawson, w.a. and bond, e.k. (1969): speech and its relation to dentistry. part iii. the effects on speech of variations in the design of dentures. dent. practit. 19, 150-156. 6. luchsinger, r. and arnold, g.e. (1965): voice — speech — language. wadsworth publishing co., belmont, california. 7. murphey, p.j. and byrd, d.l. (1966): correction of prognathia by oral surgery and orthodontic therapy. in current therapy in dentistry, goldman, h.m. et al. (eds.). c.v. mosby co., st. louis. 8. peterson, g.e. and shoup, j.f. (1966): a physiological theory of phonetics. j. speech hear. res. 9, 5-67. 9. simpson, w. (1974): the results of surgery for mandibular prognathism. brit. j. oral surg. 12, 166-176. 10. subtelny, j.d.„oya, n. and subtelny, j.d. (1972): cineradiographic study of sibilants. folia phoniat. 24, 30-35. 11. vig, p. (1967): adaptation to altered morphology — illustrated by two surgical cases. dent. practit. 17, 225-233. hearing aids — a comprehensive range comprising the latest microson directional models — unitron high level compression models. ear moulds — we specialise in ear mould technology to the great benefit of our patients — we also supply instant mould kits to schools. r tv accessories — manufactured by ourselves modern hearing aids (pty) ltd for we are at 305 rand central 165 jeppe st johannesburg tel: 37-1774 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the edinburgh masker was pioneered by the stammering research unit at edinburgh university. t h e e d i n b u r g h m a s k e r is s u p p l i e d w i t h t h e c o n s e n t o f t h e p a t i e n t ' s d o c t o r o r t h e r a p i s t . the edinburgh masker is a new electronic device designed to alleviate stammering. the needier westdene organisation (pty) limited. in association with / in medewerking met hearing and acoustic instruments (pty) ltd. lewis's hearing centre (pty) ltd. engineered acoustic products noise control needier westdene house 33 durham street, raedene, johannesburg, 2192 telex· 8-3660 p.o. box 28975 sandringham, 2131 south africa tel 45-7262,45-6113/4 cables: needlerorg die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol. 25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 33 criteria for managing audiometric data in occupational hearing conservation calum μ delaney department of logopaedics, university of cape town abstract hearing conservation programmes usually include hearing testing, although it is not always clear whether the aim of such testing is to identify individuals with a hearing disability, or those who show evidence of having been affected by noise. the requirements for hearing testing in both cases relate to three main considerations: the choice of frequencies at which hearing is assessed; the way in which this threshold data is quantified or otherwise managed; and whether this index is compared to some static limit, or to the individual's own baseline audiogram to assess hearing change. central to the assessment of hearing for the purpose of identifying individuals at risk for noise-induced hearing loss is a measure that is both sensitive and specific to the effects of noise. a case is made for a choice of frequencies around 4000 hz, the substitution of hearing loss configuration for the three-frequency average, and an emphasis on hearing change rather than status as a means of interpreting audiometric data for hearing conservation purposes. opsomming gehoor-konserverings-programme sluit gewoonlik gehoortoetsing in, alhoewel dit nie altyd duidelik is of die doel van sulke toetse is om die individue met gehoorafwykings of diegene wat deur lawaai geaffekteer is, te identifiseer nie. in beide gevalle is die vereistes vir gehoorevaluasie verwant aan drie hoofoorwegings: die keuse van frekwensies waarby gehoor getoets word; die wyse waarop die drempelgegewens kwantitatief of andersins verwerk word; en ofhierdie indeks vergelyk word met een ofander statiese limiet, of met die individu se eie basislyn-oudiogram om gehoorverandering te ondersoek. sentraal tot die evaluasie van gehoor, met die oog op identifikasie van risikogevalle virgehoorverlies wat deur lawaai veroorsaak word, is 'n meting wat beide sensitief en spesifiek is tot die gevolge van lawaai. daar bestaan oortuigende redes vir die keuse van frekwensies van om en by 4000 hz, die vervanging van die gemiddelde van drie frekwensies met gehoorkonfigurasie, en die klem op gehoorverandering eerder as gehoorstatus as wyse van interpretasie van oudiometriese data vir die doel van gehoorkonservering. most approaches to hearing conservation include the testing of hearing at some point. the aim of this is to monitor employees to identify those who show a deterioration of hearing in spite of;the wearing of hearing protection and attempts to reduce noise levels. however, the aspect of hearing deterioration that receives attention appears to suffer from a confusion of purpose that leads to a frustration of the probable intent of the hearing testing in the first place. that is, the prevention of noiseinduced hearing loss. "deterioration of hearing" may mean two things. one meaning may refer to the development of a hearing status that compromises an individual's ability to make use of his hearing for communicative and other purposes. the other meaning may be any change to hearing that may reasonably be attributed to noise exposure, regardless of what the subsequent effect of such change may be. conservation of auditory communication ability if the aim of preventing hearing deterioration is to conserve the individual's ability to use audition for communication, then measurement needs to concentrate on those hearing frequencies considered to be most important for the processing of speech information. speech sounds cover a range of frequencies from at least 100 hz to 8000 hz (fletcher, 1953), while the maximum level of the perceived spectra of summed speech ranges between 500 and 4000 hz (pascoe, 1978). due to the redundancy of information inherent in speech, normal hearing listeners are able to maintain the intelligibility of speech that has been lowpass filtered as low as 1600 hz (hirsch, reynolds & joseph, 1954), thus probably giving rise to the view that speech information in the frequency range 500 hz to 2000 hz is necessary and sufficient for adequate communication. interestingly the same study also found that eliminating all frequencies below about 1600 hz also had a similarly minimal effect on speech intelligibility. however, more recent work carried out with hearing impaired listeners (including noise-induced hearing loss) suggests that for these individuals speech information redundancy is reduced by the hearing loss and other auditory effects such as reduced frequency and temporal resolution (dreschler & plomp, 1985), as well as the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 calum μ delaney confounding effects of competing noise (suter, 1985). these subjects show increased intelligibility in noise with an increased signal bandwidth (skinner & miller, 1983), particularly with the inclusion of frequencies above 2000 hz (sullivan, allsman, nielsen, & mobley, 1992). verschuure and van benthem (1992) found that subjects with steeply sloping audiograms performed better in noise when using hearing aids with high-frequency emphasis. a consistent finding across a number of studies is that the hearing loss at 2000 and 4000 hz is the best simple predictor of speech reception and speech intelligibility in noise (abel, krever, & alberti, 1990; smoorenburg, 1990; verschuure & van benthem, 1992). in evaluating hearing thresholds it is also necessary to establish a level beyond which hearing can no longer be considered to be satisfactory (i.e., when an individual is considered to experience some "disability"), and a way to relate the thresholds at different frequencies to that level. the most convenient measure to employ in quantifying hearing loss at several frequencies is some form of simple, or occasionally weighted, average of three or more frequencies. typically these include some of the octave frequencies from 500 hz to 4000 hz, and sometimes 3000 hz and 6000 hz (e.g., british standards institution, 1976; ward, 1983). this average is then related to a level beyond which the hearing is considered abnormal and an individual is expected to begin experiencing difficulty with his hearing. this level (the "low fence") is usually 25 db or 30 db (e.g., british standards institution, 1976; united states department of labor, 1983). examination of the sabs 083-1983 code of practice (1983) which specifies recommended procedures for hearing conservation in south africa suggests that the approach to hearing measurement in this standard has much in common with the aim of conserving hearing for communication purposes outlined above. it employs the average of the three frequencies 500, 1000, and 2000 hz as a measure of threshold, and defines an impairment as occurring when this average exceeds 25 db. further, the manner of specifying risk is in terms of the probability of the above impairment occurring as a consequence of exposure to a given level of noise, taking into account the age of the individual. rather than permit an assessment of the risk posed to an individual, the procedure for assessing risk seems intended to assess whether an individual with an impairment (as defined) can reasonably be assumed to have acquired that impairment as a consequence of his noise exposure. in other words, can the "blame" for the loss be attributed to the noise rather than something else (typically age)? in evaluating this approach an additional point is worth mentioning. this is the similarity between hearing conservation as outlined above and the current approach to compensation for noise-induced hearing loss in south africa, with respect to the use made of hearing measurement. the formula for the calculation of disability specifies impairment in terms of a threshold (the average of 500, 1000, and 2000 hz) worse than 25 db. (office of the workmen's compensation commissioner, 1988 and 1992). in examining the approach to compensation it is clear that its aim is to ensure that the impairment is sufficiently disabling to warrant compensation. the point to be made is that the specification of impairment is intended to reflect disability, and is primarily concerned with hearing status (rather than change), which is similar to the approach outlined above in respect of sabs 083. it is interesting to contemplate the extent to which considerations of compensation might have influenced the approach to hearing conservation. ward (1983) and noble (1988) have commented on the arbitrary nature of the assumptions underlying formulae for compensation, and it would be unfortunate were these to have influenced procedures for conservation. identification of the effects of noise on hearing the alternative approach to monitoring hearing deterioration is to do so for the purpose of identifying individuals who are being affected by their noise exposure. this is regardless of whether or not the loss has any disabling effect on the individual's ability to use his hearing for communication and other purposes. the aim in this approach is to devise a measure that is both sensitive (generates a minimum of false negatives), and specific (generates a minimum of false positives) to the effects of noise. this raises the question of what is known about the effects of noise exposure on hearing. robinson (1987) provided a useful synthesis of the data from studies on noise-induced hearing loss available at that time. robinson's treatment of the data was extensive and what follows is a summary of the most obvious trends. plots at different frequencies of hearing threshold level against the level of noise exposure for different durations of exposure showed that within ten years of exposure to noise levels greater than 85 db(a) most studies showed evidence of some degree of threshold elevation (to >10 db hl) at 4000 hz in 50% of their subjects. at 2000 hz this was the case for 25% of subjects, while at 1000 hz there was minimal threshold elevation even at the 25th percentile. additionally the effect of noise level was greatest at short exposure durations for 4000 hz (5 to 10 years), only becoming marked (but resulting in less affected threshold levels) at increasingly longer durations for 2000 hz and 1000 hz (at approximately 20 and 30-40 years respectively). growth curves of hearing loss at 4000 hz as a function of exposure duration at levels of 90 db(a) and greater showed a rapid growth in the first ten years of exposure, levelling off thereafter. similar curves at 2000 hz and 1000 hz showed a constant growth of hearing loss with exposure duration at progressively lower rates respectively. this was true of b o t h a s c r e e n e d otologically normal population and an unscreened typical population. rate of hearing loss at 4000 hz on first entry to noise varied between 1 and 9 db/year for exposure to noise levels of 85 to 100 db(a), depending on actual level· of exposure and individual variation. finally, in a study of the effect of age on hearing loss by robinson and sutton (1979), the results showed that a noticable effect at 4000 hz only became discernable after age 30 to 35 years. apart from confirming the generally accepted fact that 4000 hz is the hearing frequency most susceptible to damage by noise, the results of robinson's work suggest that the degree of hearing loss is greatest at this frequency, it shows the greatest change at increasing the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c r i t e r i a for managing audiometric data in occupational hearing conservation 35 noise levels and it shows this change over the shortest duration i.e., most of the deterioration in hearing at 4000 hz occurs within the first ten years of exposure, and this deterioration can initially occur at a rate of up to 9 db/year. taken in conjunction with robinson and sutton's findings that the effect of age on hearing at 4000 hz is minimal before the age of 35 years, it is reasonable to suggest that change to hearing at this frequency can be attributed to noise alone in the first ten years of exposure to noise of persons under 35. monitoring of change to hearing at this frequency should therefore provide a sensitive indicator of susceptibility to noise. the next consideration is the way in which this measure should be utilised. given that noise can also affect other frequencies in the region of 4000 hz, for example 6000hz and 3000hz, an average of thresholds at several frequencies can be obtained. this is the case for example in the occupational safety and health administration (osha) hearing conservation amendment (united states department of labor, 1983) where the average threshold at 2000, 3000, and 4000 hz is used to assess susceptibility. (the frequencies used in the osha amendment are interesting, given the discussion on compensation versus conservation above. it is not clear whether the choice of frequencies is intended to reflect the effects of noise, or their effect on disability. a discussion of the amendment by suter (1984) suggested that considerations other than hearing conservation played a role in the choice finally decided upon). alternatively, a shift in threshold at any one of several frequencies can be used. the australian standard as 1269-1989 (standards association of australia, 1989) for example prescribes testing at 3000, 4000, and 6000hz as a minimum, and a threshold shift at any one of these frequencies as requiring further action. if it is a noise effect that is to be detected (rather than the presence of some form of disability) then it is important that a change to hearing at these frequencies be reacted to rather than the hearing reaching a certain pre-defined status. this is regardless of whether the change has resulted in' thresholds which are still considered normal, or whether the thresholds before exposure lay outside the normal range to begin with. the amount of deterioration of hearing considered significant is 10 db or greater in the osha amendment (united states department! of labor, 1983) and 15 db or greater in the australian' standard (standards association of australia, 1989).' at present the sabs 083 standard has no definition for "deterioration of hearing", even were either a three-frequency average or threshold shift at one of the specified test frequencies to be utilised for this purpose. audiometric configuration it is important to note that the studies incorporated in robinson's (1987) work examine the effects of noise on hearing thresholds in terms of measures of central tendency for certain populations. while this provides some information on how a noise-induced hearing loss might develop in an individual case, this data essentially obscures any individual variability that might occur. similarly, utilisation of threshold shift at isolated frequencies or in terms of an average at several frequencies may reduce the specificity of the measure by permitting hearing loss of other etiology to be misclassified as being noise-induced. examination of the audiometric data of individuals who have been exposed to noise suggests that the hearing thresholds of these individuals are poorer at 4000 hz and the frequencies in the vicinity of 4000 hz than at other frequencies. this conclusion is also indirectly supported by the data in robinson's (1987) work. importantly though the thresholds at 8000 hz, and 2000 hz and below, are noticably better than those at 4000 and 6000 hz. an important feature of utilising thresholds at the frequencies around 4000 hz as a measure of noise effect may be their level in relation to the thresholds at 2000 and 8000hz. that is, the depth of the "notch" in the audiogram at 4000 hz. thus it may be useful to consider audiometric configuration rather than averaged or isolated frequencies when developing a more specific measure of the effect of noise on hearing. as part of an earlier study (delaney, 1993) the author compared the audiometric data of just over 600 noise-exposed subjects from 6 different factories described in terms of three-frequency averages and in terms of audiometric configuration. three averages were obtained: 500, 1000 and 2000 hz; 1000, 2000, and the worst of 4000 or 6000 hz; and 2000, worst of 4000 or 6000, and 8000 hz. these averages were then grouped as falling below 26 db, between 26 db and 55 db (inclusive), and above 55 db. the audiometric configuration of each ear was categorised in terms of the depth of the "notch" as showing no noise-induced hearing loss (nihl); a possible nihl (a depth of at least 5 db on one side and 10 db on the other); and a probable nihl (a difference of at least 20 db between 2000 and 4/6000 hz and 15 db between 4/6000 and 8000 hz, or 15 db between 2000 and 4/6000 hz and 25 db between 4/6000 and 8000 hz). one of the aims was to assess the extent to which the numbers of individuals identified as having been affected by noise might differ when measured using the two different approaches. a summary of the results of this analysis for all six of the factories combined is presented in fig 1. from these results it can be seen that the number of ears showing an average hearing loss greater than 25 db (using any of the <0 cr < uj ω ξ <26 26-55 >55 <26 26-55 >55 <26 26-55 >55 .5, 1, 2khz 1, 2, 4/6khz 2, 4/6, 8khz average hearing loss (db) | | no nihl | | possible nihl i probable nihl figure 1. noise-induced hearing loss (nihl) component and severity of three-frequency-average hearing loss (<26 db, 26-55 db, and > 55 db), for three different three-frequency-averages. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings', vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 calum μ delaney three averages) is less than the number identified as having a possible or probable noise-induced component to their hearing configuration. additionally, not all individuals with a three-frequency-average hearing loss show a noise-induced component to their hearing configuration, while a large number of subjects with normal three-frequency-average hearing do. the former finding may suggest that audiometric configuration may be a more specific measure of the effect of noise, while the latter may indicate that it is more sensitive. clearly these conclusions rest on the validity of the hearing configuration criteria used to identify a noise effect. the criteria were arrived at by examining the types of hearing configurations identified by experienced audiologists as showing possible or probable noise-induced hearing loss. however, these criteria may as easily be submitted to debate as any other criteria already employed in hearing conservation. additionally a reexamination in terms of individual audiometric configuration of the raw data from some of the studies already conducted might also provide some guidelines. conclusion this discussion attempted to show that an alternative approach to the management of hearing measurement data might provide a more sensitive and specific measure of the effect of noise on hearing, thus permitting the detection of individuals being affected by noise such that conservation measures can be implemented as rapidly as possible. it was also suggested that some of the confusion that exists concerning whether considerations of disability should enter into the identification of individuals susceptible to noise, and whether considerations of compensation should be allowed to influence conservation criteria, should be avoided. choice of frequencies for assessing the effects of noise, criteria for assessing the significance of hearing change, and whether or not hearing configuration is a better means for managing audiometric data is a matter for debate. however, a careful re-examination of the philosophy and purpose behind hearing measurement in the conservation of hearing may permit the development of a practice that better serves the aim of preventing noise-induced hearing loss. references abel, s. m., krever, ε. m., & alberti, p. w. (1990). auditory detection, discrimination and speech processing in ageing, noise-sensitive and hearing-impaired listeners. scand. audiol., 19, 43-54. british s t a n d a r d s institution. (1976). bs 5330-1976: estimating the risk of hearing handicap due to noise exposure. london: british standards institution. delaney, c. m. (1993). hearing measurement for noise control: an alternative approach to assessing noise risk. in m. j. crocker & ν. i. ivanov (eds.). proceedings of the noise-93 international noise and vibration control conference (vol. 2, pp. 243-248). st petersburg, russia: interpublish. dreschler, w. α., & plomp, r. (1985). relations between psychophysical data and speech perception for hearingimpaired subjects. j. acoust. soc. am., 78, 1261-1270. fletcher, h. (1953). speech and hearing in communication. princeton: van nostrand. hirsch, i. j., r e y n o l d s , e. g., & j o s e p h , m. (1954). intelligibility of different speech materials. j. acoust. soc. am., 26, 530-538. noble. w. (1988). evaluation of hearing handicap: a critique of ward's position. audiology, 27, 53-64. office of the workmen's compensation commissioner. (1988, 1992). personal communication. pascoe, d. (1978). an approach to hearing aid selection. hear. instruments, 29, 12-16. robinson, d. w. (1987). noise exposure and hearing: a new look at the experimental data (hse contract research report no 1). london: health and safety executive. robinson, d. w., & sutton, g. j. (1979). age effect in hearing: a comparative analysis of published threshold data. audiology, 18, 320-334. skinner, m., & miller, j. (1983). amplification bandwidth and intelligibility of speech in quiet and in noise for listeners with sensorineural hearing loss. audiology, 22, 253-279. smoorenburg, g. f. (1990). on the limited transfer of information with n o i s e i n d u c e d hearing loss. acta otolaryngol, suppl. 469, 38-46. south african bureau of standards. (1983). sabs 083-1983 code of practice for the measurement and assessment of occupational noise for hearing conservation purposes (amended). pretoria: south african bureau of standards. standards association of australia. (1989). as 1269-1989: hearing conservation. sydney: standards association of australia. sullivan, j. α., allsman, c. s., nielsen, l. b., & mobley, j. p. (1992). amplification for listeners with steeply sloping, high-frequency hearing loss. ear and hearing, 13, 35-45. suter, a. h. (1984). osha's hearing conservation amendment and the audiologist. asha, 26, 39-43. suter, a. h. (1985). speech recognition in noise by individuals with mild hearing impairments. j. acoust. soc. am., 78, 887-900. united states department of labor, occupational safety and health a d m i n i s t r a t i o n . (1983). o c c u p a t i o n a l noise exposure: hearing conservation amendment. federal register, 46, 9738-9785. verschuure, j., & van benthem, p. p. g. (1992). effect of hearing aids on speech perception in noisy situations. audiology, 31, 205-221. ward, w. d. (1983). the ama/aoo formula for determination of hearing handicap. audiology, 22, 313-324. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die diagnosering van stamel as 'n vorm van ps1goneurologiese disfunksie i.e. uys. d.phil. (pretoria) dept. spraakheelkunde, universiteit van pretoria. opsomming die samestelling van 'n diagnostiese program vir stamel as 'n vorm van psigoneurologiese disfunksie word gebaseer op 'n teoretiese orientering ten opsigte van die simptomatologiese en etiologiese verband tussen hierdie twee sindrome. hierdie verband word met empiriese bevindings gestaaf. 'n omvattende diagnostiese program is saamgestel met die doel om al die verskillende simptome van psigoneurologiese disfunksie, sowel as meer spesifiek, die verbale kommunikasieprobleme van stamel, uit te lig. ten slotte word hierdie program ge-evalueer aan die hand van eksperimentele bewyse. summary the construction of a diagnostic programme for cluttering as a form of psychoneurological dysfunction is based on a theoretical orientation to the symptomatological and etiological relationship between these two syndromes. this relationship is confirmed by empirical findings. a comprehensive diagnostic programme is constructed with the purpose of eliciting all the different symptoms of psychoneurological dysfunction, as well as more specifically, the verbal communication problems of cluttering. finally this programme is evaluated according to experimental findings. wanneer 'n navorser poog om 'n sinvolle diagnostiese program vir enige afwyking saam te stel, moet so 'n program gegrond word op 'n aanvaarbare logiese teoretiese orientering. dit is egter nodig om eers 'n intensiewe studie te maak van alle relevante empiriese bevindings in verband met die afwyking wat tot so 'n teoretiese orientering kan lei. in die geval van stamel as 'n vorm van psigoneurologiese disfunksie (pnd) berus 'n sinvolle diagnostiese program op a) die bestaan van 'n simptomatologiese verband tussen stamel en pnd; b) 'n ooreenstemming in verband met die moontlike etiologiese faktore in beide gevalle; c) empiriese bewyse dat stamel wel 'n vorm van pnd is en dus as sodanig ondersoek moet word. die simptomatologiese verband tussen stamel en pnd 'n intensiewe literatuurstudie dui daarop dat pnd as 'n bree diagnostiese kategorie gesien moet word, en dat dit omvattend genoeg moet wees om voorsiening te maak vir al die uiteenlopende simptome. binne hierdie bree diagnosjourna of the south african speech and hearing association. vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) stamel en psigoneurologiese disfunksie 77 tiese kategoriee is 'n verskeidenheid van sindrome te vinde. dit is dus ook sinvol dat hierdie spesifieke simptoomkomplekse as sub-kategoriee herken en aanvaar sal word. daar bestaan egter so 'n massa publikasies oor pnd dat die student verblind word deur al die losstaande, veranderlike en veelvormige simptome en gewoonlik lei dit tot teoretiese mistasting en kliniese rondtasting. in 'n onlangse navorsingsprojek is gevind dat 'n kubernetiese beskouing veral vir die spraakterapeut van groot waarde kan wees, omdat dit nie maar weer lei tot die samestelling van 'n lys van bekende kenmerke nie, maar eerder aanduidend is van die verband tussen verskillende simptome.22 dit is juis in die kubernetika wat die verband tussen pnd en stamel gevind word. volgens navorsing wat reeds gedoen is op die gebied van stamel, is daar 'n merkwaardige ooreenkoms, op byna alle vlakke, met die beeld wat deur pnd geskep word.3' 2 0 ' 2 5 in hierdie gevalle is die spraakafwyking egter die middelpunt van die beskry wing van die simptoomkompleks, omdat die kenmerkendste of opvallendste simptome gesentreer is om spraak as kommunikasiemedium. murray11 het die kenmerkendste simptome van pnd as volg saamgevat: 1. hiperaktiwiteit 2. perseptuele — motoriese aantasting 3. emosionele labiliteit 4. algemene versteurings in ko5rdinasie 5. steurings van aandag 6. impulsiwiteit 7. steurings van geheue en denke 8. spesifieke leergestremdhede 9. steurings van spraak en gehoor 10. twyfelagtige neurologiese tekens en onreelmatighede van die elektroenkefelogram. 'n analise van die varierende simptome van stamel, veral in die lig van die bogenoemde lys van simptome, dui op die een of ander vorm van pnd. nie alle simptome, wat by verskillende stamelaars, as 'n groep voorkom, hoef as verpligtend beskou te word nie. in werklikheid kan alleenlik vier simptome as verpligtend vir die diagnose van stamel beskou word, naamlik: a) kort aandagspan en die daarmee gepaardgaande swak konsentrasievermoe; b) gebrek aan volkome bewustheid van die probleem; c) 'n buitensporige aantal herhalings in die spraak; d) 'n verswakte waarnemingsvermoe. die meeste simptome wat dikwels by stamelaars waargeneem word is egter opsioneel. hierdie simptome toon 'n groot omvang, van reseptiewe, tot integrasieen ekspressiewe probleme. simptome wat waarskynlik die opvallendste is, sluit in die ekspressiewe of motoriese manifestasies, soos buitensporige spraakspoed, artikulasie-afwykings en 'n algemene rusteloosheid of hiperaktiwiteit. die reseptiewe probleme is moeiliker waarneembaar, maar dra nogtans in 'n groot mate by tot die omvang van die probleem. hier word simptome soos tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 i.c,. uys perseptuele afwykings gevind, wat kan lei tot leesen spellingprobleme en gevolglik spesifieke skolastiese leergestremdhede. die algemeenste opsionele simptoom van stamel is waarskynlik 'n leesprobleem, wat vergelykbaar is met disleksie.25 gewoonlik word die probleem van 'n sentrale taalwanbalans of -onvermoe in verband gebring met 'n versteuring in die ontwikkeling en funksionering van die sentrale senuweesisteem. dit is op die gebied van hoer kortikale integrasie waar die merendeel van die probleme gemanifesteer word.1' 3 arnold1 be-. klemtoon die feit dat dit 'n afwyking is, wat die hoogste vlak van linguistiese formulasie en integrasie sal affekteer en bradford3 voeg daarby dat enige toestand wat die harmoniese organisasie van serebrale aktiwiteite bei'nvloed ook probleme in verband met die ontvangs en koordinasie van visuele en ouditiewe indrukke sal veroorsaak. dit sal weer die psigomotoriese stabiliteit van die persoon nadelig bei'nvloed. in hierdie geval sal die onderliggende pnd, of meer spesifiek, die swak gei'ntegreerde en onvolledige denkprosesse die oorsaak wees van die herhalende spraakpatroon, hersienings, huiwerings en stiltes, tussenwerpsels, artikulasie-afwykings, grammatikale verwarring eh leesen spellingprobleme.12 uit 'n kubernetiese studie van die simptomatologiese beskrywings van pnd aan die een kant, en stamel aan die ander kant, blyk dit dus asof daar 'n definitiewe verwantskap tussen die twee sindrome bestaan. 'n etiologiese ooreenstemming tussen stamel en pnd daar is 'n baie noue verband tussen pnd en stamel vasgestel deur eksperimentele bewyse, onder andere in verband met eienskappe van die senuweesisteem,1 3 sekere biochemiese en formakologiese aanduidings 9 ' 1 4 ' 2 6 en die omlyning van die betrokke neurofisiologiese g e b i e d e . 7 ' 1 0 ' 1 6 die volgende implikasie is dus voor die handliggend — dat in die .geval van stamel die funksionering (kwalitatief en kwantitatief) van senuwee-oordrastowwe, tesame met selen organisme sensitiwiteit, tot 'n wanbalans in die funksionering van die twee opwekkingsisteme (die retikulere en limbiese) kan bydra. die teorie, in verband met 'n verhoogde adaptasiepeil (stelling) in die retikulere formasie en 'n moontlike wanbalans in.die wisselwerking tussen die twee sisteme, is ook van toepassing gevind op stamel.22 in hierdie geval (waarskynlik as gevolg van striatum-betrokkenheid) is die kenmerkendste simptome in die .verbale kommunikasie-oordrag merkbaar. 1 9 ' 2 5 dit gebeur meestal dat ondersoekers pnd toeskryf aan kortikale wanfunksionering7 terwyl die probleem eerder in die retikulere formasie gesô ek behoort te word. dieselfde mening word gehuldig in verband met die striatum en "stamel. die geweldige omvang en wisseling van simptome kan alleenlik verklaar' word, indien die wisselwerking tussen die retikulere en limbiese sisteme in ag gene.em word. 1 empiriese bewyse in verband met stamel as 'n vorm van pnd in 'n onlangse navorsingsprojek22 is die hipotese gestel dat stamel een van . die vorms is wat pnd kan aanneem. stamel· is dan ook beskou as die manifes-. tasie van 'n, sentrale taalwanbalans,25 wat veral veruiterlik word in verbale journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) stamel en psigoneurologiese disfunksie 79 kommunikasie-afwykings. dit is toegeskryf aan afwykings in die koderingsprosesse, wat verbale uiting voorafgaan en al die verskillende kommunikasiekanale nadelig beinvloed. 'n spraakkubernetiese model is opgestel, waarvolgens 'n psigoneurologiese verklaring van die verskillende kenmerkende verbale kommunikasie-simptome moontlik is. omdat dit egter alleenlik moontlik was om die hipotese deur empiriese bevindings te bewys, is 'n omvattende, maar intensiewe interdissiplinere navorsingsprojek uitgevoer, waarin agt stamelaars, uit drie families, aan die volgende ondersoeke blootgestel is: 1. volledige neurologiese ondersoeke ' 2. elektroenkefalografiese ondersoeke 3. intelligensietoetsing 4. projeksietoetsing 5. persepsietoetsing 6. evaluasie van die kommunikasiesisteem en handeling (insluitende lees en spelling) 7. evaluasie van skolastiese prestasies 8. genetiese ontleding van familiegeskiedenisse met die oog op moontlike genetiese faktore. uit 'n kwalitatiewe analise van die toetsresultate, blyk dit dat al die agt proefpersone, in 'n mindere of meerdere mate, as persone met 'n psigoneurologiese disfunksie beskou kan word. al die verskillende simptome, wat by pnd voorkom is deur een of meer van die diagnostiese ondersoeke uitgelig by elkeen van die proefpersone. afdoende bewyse is dus gevind dat stamel 'n vorm van pnd is, met die kenmerkendste simptome gesentreer om spraak as kommunikasiemedium. kriteria vir die insluiting van toetse in die diagnostiese battery aangesien daar reeds bewys is dat stamel 'n vorm van pnd is, word die samestelling van 'n toetsbattery bepaal deur a) die vermoe van sekere toetse om al die verskillende simptome van pnd uit te lig b) die vermoe van sekere toetse om spesifiek die verbale kommunikasie abnormaliteite, kenmerkend van stamel, uit te lig. hier kan die diagnostikus gelei word deur die bevindings van vorige navorsers, tewete: russell, neuringer en goldstein17; schulman, kaspar en throne;18 francis-williams;4 joubert;7 weiss;25 goldschmidt;5 en vele ander. in die studie deur uys22 is 'n omvattende toetsbattery gebruik wat die volgende ondersoeke en toetse ingesluit het: genetiese ondersoek hierdie ondersoek dien alleenlik as aanduiding van familiale betrokkenheid. 'n tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 i.c. uys stamboom van elke familie word getrek, waarvolgens sekere afleidings, in verband met die voorkoms van stamel in families, gemaak kan word. gevalsgeskiedenis die gevalsgeskiedenis behoort so beplan te word dat dit nie alleenlik alle moontlike etiologiese faktore insluit nie, maar ook 'n evaluasie van die waargenome simptome van die pasient. aanduidings van familiale betrokkenheid kan hieruit verkry word. in bree trekke moet die volgende aspekte gedek word: voorgeboortelike geskiedenis; perinatale geskiedenis; postnatale geskiedenis; ontwikkelingsmylpale; familiegeskiedenis; akademiese prestasie; oorsake en simptome van die probleem. neurologiese evaluasie die neurologiese evaluasie kan in twee afdelings verdeel word, naamlik: 1. die elektroenkefalografiese ondersoek, waar 'n twaalf-kanaal eeg-apparaat beduidende resultate sal lewer. die bipolere metode van elektrodeplasing word aanbeveel. hoewel daar in menslike eeg-studies gewoonlik van drie eksperimentele toetse gebruik gemaak word7, is daar vir die diagnose van stamel alleenlik twee toetse noodsaaklik. photiese stimulasie vir die diagnose van epileptiese aanvalle word nie as noodsaaklik beskou nie. die „oe sluit-toets" stel die retilculere formasie en talamus in staat om alfa-sinchronisasie te bevorder en word dus aanbeveel.7 die hiperventilasie-toets veroorsaak sametrekking van die bloedvate en kan ook aanduidend wees van moontlike epilepsie.23 2. 'n lntensiewe neurologiese ondersoek behoort ingesluit te word sodat 'n kliniese evaluasie van die pasient moontlik is. in bree trekke moet die volgende aspekte hier gedek word: algemene neurologiese evaluasie; funksies van die dominante hemisfeer; funksies van die nie-dominante hemisfeer; funksies van beide hemisfere; motoriese funksies; sensoriese funksies; reflekse. psigometriese evaluasie die toetse wat in hierdie ondersoek gebruik word, kan in drie hoofgroepe verdeel word, volgens hulle vermoe om psigoneurologiese disfunksies aan te dui. intelligensie toetse. in die geval van afrikaanssprekende pasiente word die wechsler-bellevue intelligensieskaal vir volwassenes en die nuwe suid-afrikaanse intelligensieskaal vir kinders aanbeveel. vorige navorsing het reeds bewys gelewer van die diagnostiese waarde van hierdie intelligensietoetse.4'15'17'24 aan die hand van hierdie bevindings, word ook vir die diagnose van stamel 'n kwalitatiewe evaluasie van die resultate in die lig van die totale beeld aanbeveel. spesiale toetse vir visuele persepsie, visuele geheue, oog-hand-koordinasie en dus psigoneurologiese funksies: die volgende toetse kan hier van waarde wees, naamlik bender-gestalt, ellis visual designs, albasterbord-toets, grassi en draw a person. die bender gestalt en ellis visual designs kan mekaar in werklikheid vervang.2 in die toepassing van'die ellis visual designs kan die voorbeelde egter na 5 sekondes verwyder word, sodat die pasient op 'n geheuebeeld, dus integrasie van informasie, moet staatmaak. hierdie toets lewer dus journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) stamel en psigoneurologiese disfunksie 81 aanduidings van visuele geheuevermoens (integrasie), terwyl die bender gestalt veral waarneming van weergawe toets. die grassi kan as aanvullende toets vir volwassenes gebruik word, omdat die toets nie diskriminerend genoeg is vir die toepassing op kinders nie.2 1 persoonlikheidstoetse. vir persoonlikheidsmeting kan die rorschach, tematiese appersepsietoets (tat), draw a person (dap) en columbus picture analysis of growth towards maturity gebruik word. terwyl die rorschach bruikbaar is by volwassenes, is die columbus meer geskik vir kinders. al hierdie toetse het reeds bewys gelewer van 'n vermoe om psigoneurologiese disfunksies aan te d u i . 1 8 ' 6 sommige van hierdie toetse oorvleuel in 'n mate, maar kan ingesluit word waar sulke toetse vir meer as een doel gebruik word. met die uitsondering van die dap word die persoonlikheidstoetse vir die evaluering van persoonlikheid en die aanduiding van kompensatoriese meganismes gebruik. die ander toetse is weer nuttig vir die aanduiding van psigoneurologiese disfunksies. evaluasie van ouditiewe vermoens hierdie evaluasie berus op die resultate van twee groepe toetse. basiese ondersoeke is nodig vir die bepaling van die suiwertoondrempel, spraakontvangsdrempel en spraakdiskriminasievermoe. aangesien daar in die geval van pnd nie noodwendig perifere gehoorprobleme voorkom nie, kan die standaardprosedure by hierdie toetse gevolg word. spesiale ondersoeke na verskillende aspekte van ouditiewe vermoens is egter noodsaaklik in die diagnose van hierdie pasiente. aangesien daar nog 'n groot gebrek aan sulke toetse met afrikaanse norms bestaan, word die seashore measures of musical talents hiervoor aanbeveel. die fisiese dimensies van ouditiewe seine kan beskou word as die produk van frekwensie (psigies verteenwoordig as toonhoogte, terwyl die verskillende frekwensiesamestellings verteenwoordig word as timbre of toonkleur), intensiteits(psigies verteenwoordig as luidheid) en temporale faktore (psigies verteenwoordig as tydsduur, ritme en reekse). hierdie dimensies is almal ingesluit in die toets en kan aanduidend wees van psigoneurologiese disfunksies.8 hierdie toets bepaal hoofsaaklik twee aspekte van ouditiewe vermoens, naamlik: die waarneming en diskriminasie van toonhoogte, luidheid, ritme, tydsduur en timbre, sowel as ouditiewe klankgeheue. verbale kommunikasie-ondersoek aangesien daar geen gestandaardiseerde afrikaanse toetse bestaan vir die objektiewe meting van verbale kommunikasievaardighede nie, moet 'n ondersoekwyse ontwerp word wat met 'n redelike mate van sekerheid kwalitatief ontleed kan word. veral aangesien stamel hier ter sprake is, is dit noodsaaklik om hierdie aspekte meer volledig te ondersoek. 'n drieledige toets word aanbeveel vir die evaluasie van lees, skryf en spraak as 'n kommunikasiemedium. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 i.c. uys vir die leestoets kan die pasient eers 'n maklike leesstuk aangebied word waarmee sukses behaal kan word. dit sal egter die voorkoms van stamelsimptome na vore laat tree.2 5 hierdie stukke moet egter aangepas word by die pasient se ouderdom en akademiese stand. . . 'n moeilike leesstuk sal weer bepaal of enige disleksie-elemente teenwoordig is, terwyl die stamelsimptome sal afneem (met die verhoogde konsentrasie en bewustheid). die skryfvermoe kan getoets word deur van die pasient te verwag om 'n stuk van 'n gedrukte voorbeeld af te skryf, te skryf volgens diktee en 'n spontane skryftaak. hiermee word nie 'n volledige analise van die verskillende onvermoens beoog nie, maar alleenlik die vasstelling van ouditief-motoriese, visueelmotoriese en integrasie vermoens. die evaluasie van spraak as 'n kommunikasiemedium vereis nie inisieel 'n volledige linguistiese analise nie — eerder is van waarde 'n stel toetse om die verskillende vlakke van abstrahering (integrasie) en proposisionering te onderskei en sodoende stamelsimptome uit te lig. so kan die volgende take van die pasient verwag word: die beskrywing van 'n prent; storievertelling volgens 'n strokiesprent; storievertelling volgens een prent; spontane spraak. hierdie diagnostiese battery word as voldoende in omvang beskou, mits die ondersoekprosedure aan die volgende vereistes voldoen: a) die resultate moet 'n getroue weergawe van die pasient se beste pogings wees b) dit moet nie die pasient uitput nie . c) aangesien dit 'n multidissiplinere diagnostiese program is, behoort deskundiges op elke gebied die toetse af te neem. d) die omstandighede moet voldoen aan die vereistes van elke toets. evaluasie van die diagnostiese program hierdie diagnostiese program is getoets aan die hand van agt pasiente se resultate. in tabel 1 kan die vermoe van hierdie toetse, om al die verskillende simptome van pnd uit te lig, duidelik gesien word. die syfers in die kolomme is 'n aanduiding van hoeveel pasiente (uit agt) die spesifieke simptoom openbaar het. 'n evaluasie van die diagnostiese vermoe van die verskillende toetse, het die volgende gegewens aan die lig gebring: 1. die neurologiese ondersoeke het veral gedui op perseptueel-motoriese versteurings, twyfelagtige neurologiese tekens, en, in die geval van "die elektroenkefalogram ondersoek, onreelmatighede. 2. die intelligensietoetse is van groot diagnostiese waarde. perseptueel-motoriese versteurings, emosionele probleme, twyfelagtige neurologiese tekens en integrasieversteurings (denke en geheueversteurings) het veral uit hierdie toetsresultate geblyk. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) stamel en psigoneurologiese disfunksie 83 simptome van psigoneurologiese disfunksie toetse h ip er ak ti w it ei t | pe rs ep tu ee lm ot or ie se aa n ta st in g e m os io ne le la bi lit ei t k ob rd in as ie ve rs te ur in gs a an da gs ve rs te ur in gs im pu ls iw it ei t d en ke -e n ge he ue . ve rs te ur in gs sp es if ie ke le er ge st re m dh ed e | sp ra ak ve rs te ur in gs g eh oo rve rs te ur in gs t w yf el ag ti ge ne ur ol og ie se te ke ns o nr ee lm at ig he de va n di e e e g . eeg 5 neurologiese ondersoek 1 8 3 2 2 1 8 wechsler/ n.s.a.i.s. 7 2 7 5 3 bender 5 7 3 6 2 4 1 8 ellis 3 7 2 3 2 3 5 8 dap. 3 4 7 3 1 1 7 grassi 1 1 albas terbord 7 2 1 1 5 5 rorschach/ columbus 1 5 4 1 2 4 tat. 7 2 2 oudiometriese ondersoek seashore 8 8 leestoets i 2 3 1 3 6 7 leestoets ii 3 1 3 6 5 skriftoets i 8 6 1 5 8 skriftoets ii 8 6 1 5 8 2 skriftoets iii 1 8 '7 1 5 8 spraaktoets i 1 8 spraaktoets ii 2 1 8 8 spraaktoets iii 2 1 8 8 spraaktoets iv 7 4 8 2 8 8 tabel 1: simptoomverspreiding na aanleiding van die toetsresultate. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 84 i.c. uys 3. toetse vir visuele vermoens is veral van diagnostiese waarde ten opsigte van perseptueel-motoriese versteurings, motoriese koordinasieprobleme, aandagsversteurings en twyfelagtige neurologiese tekens. dit blyk egter dat nie alle toetse vir visuele vermoens diagnosties ewe goed is nie. die grassi het opmerklik minder simptome aangedui as die ander en in 'n mindere mate ook die dap. die bender en ellis het geblyk die beste diagnostiese waarde te he. 4. persoonlikheidstoetse dui veral op emosionele versteurings en kompensasiemeganismes. in 'n mindere mate kan hieruit ook inligting in verband met integrasieprobleme en aandagsversteurings verkry word. die rorschach en columbus-toetse toon 'n beter diagnostiese vermoe as die tat. 5. toetse vir ouditiewe vermoens. die seashore toets is baie waardevol vir die bepaling van ouditiewe waarnemings-, diskriminasieen geheuefunksies. 6. leestoetse, veral waar dit verbind word met die ouditiewe en visuele funksies onderskeidelik, is aanduidend van integrasie-vermoens, spesifieke leergestremdhede en ook verskillende spraakafwykings. 7. die skriftoetse het veral aanduidings gelewer van perseptueel-motoriese versteurings, ko5rdinasie en integrasieprobleme, asook spesifieke leesonvermoens. 8. die spraaktoetse het, as gevolg van die gradering, gedui op integrasieprobleme, aandagsversteurings en hiperaktiwiteit. uiteraard het hierdie toetse die meeste inligting verskaf in verband met die voorkoms, verspreiding en aard van die verskillende spraakafwykings. alhoewel hierdie diagnostiese program dus toereikend blyk te wees vir die diagnose van stamel as 'n vorm van pnd, word dit in die vooruitsig gestel dat meer objektiewe metingstoetse van groter nut sal wees, veral met die oog op kwantitatiewe analises van resultate. daar is 'n groot behoefte aan gestandaardiseerde toetse met suid-afrikaanse norms. verwysings 1. arnold, g.e., (1970): an attempt to explain the causes of cluttering ' with the l.l.m.m. theory. folia phoniatrica. vol. 22, no. 4-5, 247-260. 2. aron, a.m. (1972): minimal cerebral dysfunction in childhood. j. comm. dis., vol. 5, no. 2, bl. 142-153. 3. bradford, d., (1970): cluttering. folia phoniatrica. vol. 22, no. 4-5, bl. 272-279. 4. francis-williams, j., (19 70): children with specific learning difficulties. pergamon press, n.y. 5. goldschmidt, p., (1970): a casuistic model of a logopedic assessment in private practice. folia phoniatrica. vol. 22, no. 4-5, bl. 289-300. 6. gunzberg, h.c., (1955): scope and limitations of the goodenough drawing test method in clinical work with mental defectives. j. clin., psycho., vol. 11, bl. 8-15. ! 7. joubert, m.j., (1969): psychoneurological dysfunction in school-going . children. ongepubliseerde proefskrif, universiteit van pretoria. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) stamel en psigoneurologiese disfunksie 85 8. katz, j. (ed.), (1972): handbook of clinical audiology. the williams & wilkins co. baltimore. 9. krakowski, a.j., (1956): amitriptyline in treatment of hyperkinetic children: a double blind study. psychosomatics. vol. 6, bl. 355. 10. moruzzi, g. & magoun, h.w., (1949): brain stem reticular formation & activation of the e.e.g. electroencephalography & clinical neurophysiology. vol. 1, bl. 455. 11. murray, c.h. de c., (1969): verslag van die komitee van ondersoek na die opvoeding van kinders met minimale breindisfunksie. dept. van hoer onderwys, die statsdrukker, pretoria. 12. mussafia, m., (1970): various aspects of cluttering. folia phoniatrica. vol. 22, no. 4-5, bl. 337-346. 13. nebylitsyn, v., (1972): the basic properties of the nervous system. soviety science review. maart, bl. 113-120. 14. op't hof, j., (1973): die genetiese benadering tot leergestremdhede met spesiale verwysing na spesifieke leergestremdhede. referaat gelewer bu doe stogtomgslpmgres vam doe s.a. vereniging vir leeren opvoedingsmoeilikhede. 15. reitan, r.m. & boll, t.j., (1973): neurophysiological correlates of minimal brain dysfunction. annals new york academy sciences. vol. 205, bl. 65-88. 16. routtenberg, α., (1968): the two-arousal hypothesis: reticular formation & limbic system. psychological review. vol. 75, no. 1, bl. 51-80. 17. russell, e.w., neuringer, c. & goldstein, g., (1970): assessment of brain damage. a neuropsychological key approach. wiley-interscience, n.y. 18. schulman, j.l., kaspar, j.c. & throne, f.m., (1965): brain damage & behaviour. a clinical-experimental study. charles c. thomas springfield, iii. 19. seeman, m., (1965): sprachstorungen beikindern. v.e.b. verlag. volk und gesundheit, berlin-jena. 20. seeman, m., (1970): relations between motorics of speech and general motor ability in clutterers. folia phoniatrica. vol. 22, no. 4-5, bl. 376-380. 21. steyn, d., (1973): kliniese sielkundige, dept. spraakwetenskap, spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria. persoonlike onderhoude. 22. uys, i.c., (1974): psigoneurologiese disfunksie: 'n tipering volgens sekere spraakpatologiese verskynsels. ongepubliseerde proefskrif, universiteit van pretoria. 23. van der merwe, h.p. (1973): persoonlike mededeling. neuroloog, h.f. verwoerd hospitaal, pretoria. 24. wechsler, d., (1949): manual wechsler intelligence scale for children. the psychological corporation, n.y. 25. weiss, d.a. (1964): cluttering. speech foundation series. prenticehall inc., englewood cliffs, n.j. 26. wender, p.h., (1971): minimal brain dysfunction in children. wileyinterscience, n.y. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acoustimed (pty.) ltd. (edms.) bpk. s o u t h a f r i c a n m a n u f a c t u r e r s of * audiometers and sound proof rooms * speech trainers and group systems * hearing aids and components we provide a repair service for all makes of hearing aids and manufacture all types of earmoulds including clear soft moulds. please w r i t e or call: acoustimed (pty) ltd., 302 sandowim centre, maud st., sandown 2196, johannesburg. p.o. box 782131, sandton 2146 radio telephone: dial 213361 ask for code 564, channel 4. journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die m o e d e r k i n d v e r h o u d i n g by kinders met 'n gesplete lip en/of v e r h e m e l t e en minimale-breindisfunksie ronel steenekamp b . a . ( l o g ) ( p r e t . ) alberton kinderleidingkliniek, privaatsak x2805, alberton, 1450. opsomming die doel van hierdie studie is om die invloed van 'n fisies waarneembare afwyking en 'n fisies nie-waarneembare afwyking op die moeder-kind-verhouding na te gaan. die moeder-kind-verhouding by twee verskillende afwykings nl. gesplete lip en/of verhemelte en minimale-brein-disfunksie is bestudeer. die verhouding tussen die moeder en haar kind is 'n kritiese faktor in die algemene ontwikkeling van die kind. die kwaliteit van die verhouding kan 'n invloed he op latere verhoudinge wat die kind in sy lewe aangaan. daar is gevind dat pre-, perien postnatalegebeure hierdie verhouding kan bei'nvloed. 'n werkhipotese is gestel nl. dat die moeder-kind-verhouding betekenisvol swakker is by die ondersoekgroepe as die van die kontrolegroep. ten einde die hipotese te bewys is "the mother-child-relationship evaluation"-vraelys ingevul deur moeders met kinders met die genoemde afwykings. daar is egter gevind dat daar nie 'n beduidende verskil is in die moeder-kind-verhouding van moeders met kinders wat 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte het nie. moeders met kinders met minimale-brein-disfunksie, is egter geneig om did kinders te verwerp. summary the aim of this study is to investigate the influence of a physically observable defect and a non-observable defect on the mother-child relationship. mother-child relationships of two defects viz. cleft lip and/or palate and minimal brain dysfunction were studied. the relationship between a mother and her child is a critical factor in the overall development of the child. the quality of the relationship can influence the child's subsequent relationships. it has been found that preperiand postnatal events can influence this relationship. a working hypothesis was postulated viz. that the mother-child relationship is significantly weaker in the experimental groups than in the control group. to prove this hypotheses, the "mother-child-relationship evaluation" questionnaire was completed by mothers of children with the mentioned defects. however, it was found that there is no significant difference in the mother-child relationship in mothers of children with a cleft lip and/or palate. however, mothers of children with minimal brain dysfunction are inclined to reject these children. die verhouding wat bestaan tussen die moeder en haar kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte of haar kind met minimale-breindisfunksie is van teoretiese en praktiese belang. die moeder-kindverhouding is 'n kritiese komponent in die aanpassing van elke kind op 'n latere stadium van sy lewe. die verhouding het egter ook 'n invloed op die kind se spraak en taalontwikkeling. enige teoretiese begrip van die kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte of minimale-breindisfunksie en sy ontwikkeling, moet 'n studie insluit van die moeder se reaksie op die inisiele kennisneming van haar kind se probleem, hoe hierdie reaksie verander het met verloop van tyd, hoe hierdie reaksie the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) moeder-kind verhouding 109 bygedra het tot die kind se spraakvaardigheid asook die algemene aanpassing van die kind. die toepassing van hierdie kennis kom tot uiting in die beplanning van 'n remedierende program vir die kind. die spraakterapeut moet bepaal wat die houding van die moeder is t.o.v. haar kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte of minimale-brein-disfunksie en of hierdie verhouding 'n invloed het op prognose of die spraakprobleem teenwoordig. hierdie verhouding moet dus in ag geneem word by die opstel van 'n terapieprogram. die doel van hierdie studie is dus om die invloed van 'n fisies waarneembare afwyking en 'n fisies nie-waarneembare afwyking op die moeder-kind-verhouding na te gaan. die vraag is of die moeder-kindverhouding by die kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte of minimale-brein-disfunksie verskil in vergelyking met die moeder-kindverhouding by normale kinders. die verhouding tussen die moeder en kind is essensieel vir die voortbestaan en ontwikkeling van die kind. hierdie verbintenis tussen die moeder en haar kind is waarskynlik die sterkste verbintenis wat die mens aangaan. die aanvanklike verhouding is 'n kritiese faktor in die algemene ontwikkeling van die kind. meer spesifiek het dit 'n invloed op die intellektuele en verbaal-linguistiese ontwikkeling. die sterkte en kwaliteit van hierdie verhouding kan ook 'n invloed he op die latere verhoudings wat die kind in sy lewe aangaan. die houding van die moeder kan ook die kind se aanvaarding van homself en dus sy selfbeeld bei'nvloed. tydens die periode van swangerskap vind daar baie emosionele skommelinge plaas, positief en negatief. die moeder moet leer om haar verantwoordelikhede te verplaas van haarself na haar kind. sy moet tot aanvaarding kom van die swangerskap en besef dat sy moeder word. sy moet identifiseer met die fetus as integrale deel van haarself, maar ook van die fetus as individu. hierdie aanvaarding ontstaan tydens die beweging van die fetus in die uterus. die perinatale periode is 'n periode van uiterste sielkundige kwesbaarheid a.g.v. die fisiese en psigiese veranderinge wat plaasvind by die moeder en haar kind. die geboorte is die klimaks van die swangerskap. die moeder sien uit daarna om haar kind te aanskou. die geboorte is die finale resultate van haar konflikte a.g:v. angs en vrees vir swangerskap en geboorte. gebeure tydens die geboorteproses kan die moeder be'invloed in die ontwikkeling van 'n verhouding met haar kind. indien dit nie 'n normale geboorte was nie, kan die moeder skuldig voel omdat sy beheer verloor het tydens die geboorteproses. postnatale depressie kan hieraan toegeskryf word. die moeder benut dus nie die periode direk na geboorte om 'n verhouding met haar kind te ontwikkel nie weens die invloed van verdowingsmiddels op haar en haar kind en postnatale ongerief. die tydperk direk na geboorte word deur verskeie outoriteite as 'n uiters kritiese periode beskou in die ontwikkeling van die die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 110 ronel steenekamp moeder-kind-verhouding. die periode is baie kort en 'n komplekse interaksie bind die moeder en haar kind saam. tydens die periode verkry die moeder oogkontak met haar kind, sy streel en soen hom, borsvoeding vind plaas en sy praat met die baba ten einde sy aandag te behou. gedurende die eerste paar weke na geboorte word interaksie hoofsaaklik deur die huil van die baba bewerkstellig. die huil bring die moeder in die posisie waar visuele, olfaktoriese en taktiele stimuli van waarde is. glimlag word ook gebruik om die moeder naby die baba te hou. 'n glimlag is belonend vir die moeder, sy bly dus naby die baba en voorsien in sy behoeftes. glimlag het vertroeteling tot gevolg en versterk vertroeteling, daarom is dit een van die belangrikste reaksies in die moeder-kind-verhouding. vokalisasie word ook as 'n veranderlike in hierdie komplekse moeder-kind-interaksie beskou. tydens interaksie is die moeder altyd besig om met haar baba te praat of te vokaliseer. dit maak dus deel uit van haar stimulasie. haar houding sal die hoeveelheid spontane ,,gesprekvoering" bepaal. die baba se reaksie op haar vokalisasie sal ook bepaal hoeveel sy met die baba gaan praat. die moeder praat nie net met haar baba nie, maar moedig hom aan om ook met haar te praat. klaus & kennel3 voel dat kommunikasie beweging insluit. die baba beweeg in ooreenstemming met die volwassene se spraak. die moeder sal dus met die baba praat om reaksie uit te lok. sosiale interaksie ontwikkel geleidelik uit hierdie vroee verhouding met die moeder, namate die huil van die baba afneem en die tyd wat hy wakker is toeneem. daar is dus nou meer geleentheid tot speelinteraksie. een van die sterkste faktore in vroee sosiale interaksie is spontane spel. die baba sal baie meer babbel tydens spel, wat by die moeder die gevoel laat dat dit wat sy doen wel iets beteken. indien die moeder by die kind is in vreemde situasies, tussen vreemde mense, sal die kind weg beweeg van haar en die nuwe omgewing ondersoek. dit is belangrik dat die kind die moeder verlaat en leer omtrent die groter wereld. hy moet dus onafhanklik raak om te funksioneer as volwassene in die samelewing. die kind moet in interaksie verkeer met die lewelose omgewing en met ander lede van sy sosiale omgewing behalwe die moeder. die moeder-kind-verhouding moet nie hierdie eksplorasie verhoed nie. symonds9 onderskei tussen vier houdings wat by die moeder aahwesig kan wees t.o.v. haar kind nl. verwerping, oortoegeeflikheid, oorbeskerming en aanvaarding. aanvaarding teenoor verwerping word beskou as die fundamentele elemente van die verhouding. dit is die mate van aanvaarding wat die ander aspekte van die moeder se gedrag bepaal. aanvaarding of verwerping van die kind word deur verskillende outoriteite as 'n primere faktor in die etiologie van spraakprobleme beskou. | die geboorte van 'n kind is die hoogtepunt van die moeder se verwagtinge en omsluit haar toekomsdrome, daarom is die geboorte the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) moeder-kind verhouding 111 van 'n kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte 'n skok vir elke moeder. die kind verskil van die beeld wat die moeder opgebou het tydens swangerskap. die voorkoms van die kind bei'nvloed die proses van eksternalisasie van 'n interne beeld wat gedurende die nege maande gegroei het. in plaas daarvan om die kind waarvan sy gedroom het te baar, het sy die een gebaar wat sy gevrees het sy sal baar. die gevolg kan verwerping of oorbeskerming wees. die moeder word dus oorweldig deur 'n konflik van positiewe en negatiewe gevoelens wat bewus of onbewus kan wees. verskillende vrae sal by die moeder ontstaan t.o.v. die uiterlike voorkoms van die kind, reaksie van die vader, familie en vriende, die oorsaak en oorerflikheid van die afwyking en finansiele probleme. die reaksie van die moeder op die vrae word bepaal deur die wyse waarop dit beantwoord word. hierdie antwoorde bepaal die langtermyn probleme waarvoor sy nou te staan kom. voor optimale ontwikkeling van 'n moeder-kind-verhouding kan begin, moet die moeder haar skuld-, angsen onsekerheidsgevoelens verwerk. die kritiese periode word dus nie benut nie a.g.v. angs omtrent die kind se toestand, gevoelens van mislukking en hulpeloosheid en fisiese skeiding van die moeder en haar kind. sommige moeders kan nie tot aanvaarding kom van die kind met sy afwyking nie. hulle verkeer vir maande in 'n toestand van selfverwyt en bejammering. as reaksie op haar negatiewe gevoelens teenoor haar kind, ontstaan skuldgevoelens wat die verhouding met haar kind van die begin af bei'nvloed. dit het oorbeskerming tot gevolg. die feit dat meeste babas met 'n gesplete lipen/of verhemelte nie geborsvoed kan word nie, dra ook by tot die emosionele konflik van die moeder. die baba moet versigtig gevoed word, in 'n spesiale posisie gehou word, meer toegelaat word om winde op te breek en meer gereeld gevoed word. die moeder moet spesiale voedingsmetodes gebruik wat die abnormaliteit van die kind beklemtoon. voeding is nie meer 'n tyd van plesier en vertroeteling nie. die herstel van die lip en prepalatale spleet sal plaasvind gedurende die eerste drie maande na geboorte en voor die kind drie jaar oud is, sal die palatale spleet herstel word. voordat die kind skool toe gaan, sal hy reeds twee, drie of vier operasies ondergaan het om die spleet bevredigend te herstel. elke operasie impliseer skeiding van die moeder, narkose, pyn van die orale omgewing en tydelike beperking van die bewegings van die hande en arms om manipulering van die operatiewe areas te voorkom. skeidingsangs ontstaan by die kind en die moeder tydens hierdie operatiewe tydperk. die moeder toon ook vrees t.o.v. operatiewe prosedures en gevolge. die moeder-kind-verhouding is die primere area van versteuring. hospitalisasie onderbreek roetine en die lewenssiklus van die kind. die spraak en psigolinguistiese vermoens van die kind is ook dikwels afwykend wat die gevolg is van orale onvermoens, maar ook weens 'n die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 112 ronel steenekamp gebrek aan stimulasie en die feit dat die kritiese periode nie ten voile benut is nie. die moeder is bewus daarvan dat spraakprobleme voorkom by die kind en daarom moedig sy nie die kind aan om te praat nie. daar word laer vereistes t.o.v. sy spraaken taalgebruik gestel. sommige moeders kritiseer van die begin af die kind se spraakpogings, wat by die kind die gevoel laat dat sy spraak onverstaanbaar is. hy kan ook voel dat hy nie sy moeder tevrede stel nie. hierdie spraakprobleme beklemtoon ook die kind se andersheid by die moeder en later by die kind self. die kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte vereis voortdurende gespesialiseerde aandag en ondersoeke. dit vereis finansieel baie van die ouers ten koste van ander behoeftes. die moeder ondervind dus uiterste angs t.o.v. haar kind se probleme tydens die groeitydperk. die emosionele omgewing is dus gestrem van geboorte af en bly dikwels abnormaal tydens die opgroei van die kind. die moeder ervaar verskillende houdings van familie, vriende en vreemde persone weens die andersheid van haar kind, wat die normale ontwikkeling van 'n verhouding met haar kind strem. precht7 beweer dat abnormaliteite tydens swangerskap en geboorte 'n uiters belangrike faktor is in die etiologie van neurologiese abnormaliteite. hy vind dat tot 68% van kinders waar probleme preof perinataal voorgekom het, neurologiese abnormaliteite vertoon. volgens precht het 70% van hierdie neurologies gestremde kinders spiertremors getoon van alle liggaamspiere, asook hiperaktiwiteit, gedragsprobleme en emosionele onstabiliteit. hulle het ook op 'n later stadium leerprobleme getoon op skool. die geboorte van 'n premature baba kan beskou word as 'n psigososiale krisis wat 'n besliste emosionele invloed op die moeder het. sy moet eerstens die moontlikheid aanvaar dat sy haar kind kan verloor deur die dood. tweedens moet sy die gevoel dat sy nie 'n normale kind kan baar nie, aanvaar en verwerk. derdens moet sy na die inisiele skeiding, tydens die kritiese periode, 'n verhouding met haar kind opbou en vierdens moet sy aanpas by die bepaalde ontwikkeling van 'n premature baba. meeste van die moeders ervaar dus gevoelens van angs en skuld. die feit dat die moeder van haar kind geskei word, verhoog haar skuldgevoelens. die skeiding laat haar voel dat sy nie in staat is om na haar eie kind om te sien nie. die minimale-brein-disfunksie-sindroom, wat waarskynlik meestal die gevolg is van preof postnatale komplikasies, het 'n besliste invloed op die gedragspatrone van die kind, maar nie in so 'n mate dat die moeder professionele hulp verkry nie. so 'n kind kan die moeder egter uitermatig irriteer, wat oorangstigheid1, oorbeskerming of verwerping van die kind tot gevolg kan he. | die minimale-brein-disfunksie-sindroom kan in twee vorme voorkom nl. die hipokinetiese kind, wat ook dikwels hipotonies is, apaties, stadig en min drink en swak reageer op stimuli. aangesien hierdie the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) moeder-kind verhouding 113 kind minder huil as die normale kind, beskou die moeder hom as 'n "soet" kind. daar ontstaan dikwels by die moeder 'n vrees dat sy die slap baba kan seermaak. aangesien die kind swak reageer op stimuli, sal die moeder hierdeur bei'nvloed word. die moeder sal minder stimuli aan die kind bied, omdat hy minder stimuli bied en swak reageer op haar stimuli. die baba se slaperigheid, huiwerige bewegings en reaksies en uitdrukkinglose gesig, kan die ontwikkeling van 'n normale moeder-kind-verhouding bei'nvloed. meestal is hierdie kinders egter hiperaktiewe kinders, hipertonus kom voor, die baba huil baie, 'n lae drempel kom voor t.o.v. die mororefleks, hy word moeilik wakker, maar raak ook weer moeilik aan die slaap. die kind word moeilik getroos as hy begin huil. 'n lae frekwensie tremor word getoon. hy skrik maklik en kom angstig voor. voedingsprobleme word getoon. a.g.v. 'n swak suigrefleks en braking. baie angs is by die moeders teenwoordig. hulle voel dat hulle die kind mishandel en ontwikkel skuldgevoelens. die kinders is dus nie maklike deelnemers aan die moeder-kind-verhouding nie en die ontwikkeling van 'n gebalanseerde verhouding word bei'nvloed. precht7 het gevind dat die moeders nie bewus was daarvan dat hierdie simptome tekens is van minimale-brein-disfunksie nie. hierdie gedrag het by hulle kommer gewek en hulle gei'rriteer. hulle het hierdie simptome as deel van die kind se temperament en persoonlikheid beskou. die gedrag het die moeder se gedrag en vertroeteling van haar kind bei'nvloed. die moeders het nie 'n harmonieuse, positiewe houding teenoor hulle kinders geopenbaar nie. die moeders was bekommerd of hulle die kind korrek hanteer. die kind se gedrag het nie die moeder tevrede gestel nie en die gevolg was dat die moeder onrustig gevoel het. fisiese uitputting kom by die moeders voor, aangesien die kind uiters baie energie vereis van die moeder weens sy swak eeten slaappatroon en ontydige gehuil. die moeder is dus ongeduldig en gei'rriteerd wat haar verhouding met hierdie kind, sowel as ander kinders in die gesin en die vader bei'nvloed. dit lei tot rusies en wrywing. vyandigheid kan ontstaan teenoor die kind wat lei tot oortoegeeflikheid of verwerping. infantiliteit van die kind veroorsaak dat hy afhanklik bly van die moeder en wat daartoe lei dat sy die kind kan oorbeskerm. die moeder se emosionele houding, vyandigheid, angstigheid en depressie word oorgedra aan die kind as verwerping. die kind se reaksies is dan 'n terugvoering van die moeder se houding. verwerping word getoon in die kind se soeke na liefde en pogings om aandag te kry in so 'n mate dat hy nie belangstelling ontwikkel in die buitewereld nie. hierdie kinders toon kenmerkende ontwikkelingsprobleme, wat taalontwikkeling insluit, waarskynlik a.g.v. 'n vertraging in die maturasie van die dele van die brein wat betrokke is by motoriese koordinasie en taal. hierdie spraak en taalprobleme word getoon in vertraagde ontwikkeling, artikulasie afwykings, hakkel en stamel. sy die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 114 ronel steenekamp taalontwikkeling is egter nie net stadig nie, maar manifesteer deur sy lewe in sy lees en spelprobleme en swak woordeskat. hierdie probleme kan egter ook toegeskryf word aan 'n gebrek aan stimulasie tydens die kritiese periode en die houding van die moeder teenoor haar kind. uit hierdie literatuurstudie kan daar dus tot die gevolgtrekking gekom word, dat moeder-kind-verhoudings waarskynlik versteur kan wees by moeders met kinders wat 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte of minimale-brein-disfunksie het. tydens die ontwikkeling van hierdie versteurde moeder-kind-verhouding is die kind se linguistiese en taalontwikkeling ook bei'nvloed. spraak en taalafwykings by hierdie kinders kan dus ook aan hierdie versteurde moeder-kind-verhouding toegeskryf word, afgesien van organiese afwykings teenwoordig. eksperimentele ontwerp 1 . fflpotese-stelling 'n nulhipotese-stelling word t.o.v. elke groep gestel. hq daar is geen beduidende verskil tussen die moeder-kindverhouding van die onderskeie ondersoekgroepe en die kontrole groep nie. 'n werkhipotese word hierteenoor gestel. hi die moeder-kind-verhouding is betekenisvol swakker by die onderskeie ondersoekgroepe as die van die kontrolegroep. h 2 die moeder-kind-verhouding van die ondersoekgroepe is betekenisvol beter as die van die kontrolegroep. 2 . doelstellings die doel van hierdie ondersoek is tweeledig nl. — die bepaling van die aard van die moeder-kind-verhouding by moeders met 'n kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte of minimale-brein-disfunksie teenoor moeders met normale kinders. — die berekening van die verband tussen bepaalde gesinsveranderlikes en die moeder-kind-verhouding. 3 . proefpersone kriteria van seleksie κ (a) die kontrolegroep / / die moeders in die kontrolegroep mag geen kind he met enige fisiese abnormaliteite of spraak-probleme nie. die swangerskap en geboorte van die betrokke kind moet relatief normaal verloop het, geen voedingsprobleme moet voorgekom het nie, 'n relatief normale ontwikkelingsverloop moet voorgekom het en geen gedragsafwykings mag getoon word nie, aangesien hierdie faktore is wat die normale ontwikkeling van die moeder-kind-verhouding kan bei'nvloed. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) moeder-kind verhouding 115 (b) die gesplete lip en/of verhemeltegroep die moeders moet een of meer kinders he met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte met gevolglike sekondere probleme soos voedingsprobleme, spraak-afwykings, emosionele probleme, sosiale probleme, ortodontiese probleme en afwykings van gehoor. (c) die minimale-brein-disfunksiegroep die moeders moet een of meer kinders he met minimale-breindisfunksie d.w.s. die kind moet 'n preof perinatale geboortegeskiedenis toon. daar moet faktore aanwesig wees soos voedingsprobleme, die kind moet 'n huilerige baba gewees het, wat min geglimlag en vokaliseer het, met 'n vertraagde ontwikkelingsgeskiedenis. die kind moet hiperof hipo-aktief wees, gedragsprobleme toon, kort aandagspan asook emosionele onstabiliteit. sover moontlik is die kind deur 'n spesialis as 'n kind met minimale-brein-disfunksie gediagnoseer. die kind moet egter ook 'n spraakafwyking toon soos artikulasieof taalafwykings, hakkel, stamel of disleksie. omskrywing van die groepe twee ondersoekgroepe is gebruik nl. — twintig moeders, waarvan vyftien afrikaanssprekend was en vyf engelssprekend, met 'n kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte. twaalf van hierdie kinders was seuns en agt dogters. — vier en twintig moeders, waarvan vyftien afrikaanssprekend was en nege engelssprekend, met 'n kind met minimale-breindisfunksie plus 'n spraakafwyking. twintig van hierdie kinders was seuns en vier dogters. een kontrolegroep is gebruik bestaande uit— — vier en dertig moeders, waarvan sewe en twintig afrikaanssprekend en sewe engelssprekend was, met normale kinders d.w.s. kinders sonder 'n fisiese-.of spraakgebrek. sestien van hierdie kinders was seuns en agtien dogters. verskeie gesinsveranderlikes is sover moontlik konstant gehou by al drie groepe. (tabel 1.) die gemiddelde ouderdom van die moeders, van al drie groepe, was 35 jaar, die gemiddelde aantal kinders wat die moeders gehad het, was drie kinders. die gemiddelde geboorte-orde van hierdie kinders was twee en die gemiddelde ouderdom van die kinders was sewe jaar. die sosio-ekonomiese klas van hierdie groepe is ook sover moontlik konstant gehou deur besonderhede te verkry van die beroep van die vader, sowel as die van die moeder asook die woonbuurt waar hulle woonagtig is. die moeders kom oorwegend uit 'n gemiddelde-sosio-ekonomiese klas. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 116 ronel steenekamp tabel i: g e s i n s v e r a n d e r l i k e s b e t r o k k e by die o n d e r s k e i e g r o e p e . standaard τ grade van beduidenveranderlike gemiddeld afwyking waarde vryheid heidsvlak gemiddelde ouderdom van moeder: kontrole 33,70 5,885 gesplete lip 34,30 5,292 0 , 3 7 52 0,712 m b d 35,33 3,931 1 , 1 8 56 0,243 gemiddelde aantal kinders: kontrole 2,50 0,896 gesplete lip 2,70 1,129 0 , 7 2 52 0,476 m b d 3,16 1,129 2 , 5 0 56 0,015* gemiddelde geboorte-orde: kontrole 1,73 0,898 gesplete lip 2,00 1,170 0 , 9 3 52 0,355 m b d 2,16 1,239 1 , 5 4 56 0,130 gemiddelde ouderdom van kind: kontrole 6,91 2,800 gesplete lip 6,90 4,621 0,01 52 0,991 mbd 7,37 3,005 0 , 6 0 56 0,550 *beduidend op die 5% vlak 4 . toetsmateriaal omskrywing van die toetsmateriaal die toetsmateriaal bestaan uit 'n vraelys nl. "the mother-childrelationship-evaluation"-vraelys. die vraelys is opgestel deur robert m. r o t h 8 en gepubliseer deur die western psychological services, california. die vraelys verskaf 'n meetinstrument vir die bepaling van moeders se houdings teenoor hulle kinders. dit evalueer die moeder-kind-verhouding objektief in terme van vier houdingskale te wete: aanvaarding (a)—12 items oorbeskerming (ob) — 12 items oortoegeeflikheid (ot) — 12 items verwerping (v) — 12 items twee pole word weergegee in hierdie vraelys nl. aanvaarding teenoor nie-aanvaarding. oorbeskerming, verwerping en oortoegeeflikheid is vorme van nie-aanvaarding. die houding van die moeder kan 'n positiewe en negatiewe karakter toon. dit is moontlik dat 'n moeder vyandig voel teenoor haar kind en die kind aanvaar. i hierdie vraelys is vertaal in afrikaans, taalkundig nagesien en deur 'n sielkundige aan die universiteit van pretoria goedgekeur as 'n betroubare weergawe van die oorspronklike. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) moeder-kind verhouding 117 kriteria van keuse die "mother-child-relationship-evaluation"-vraelys (mcre) is gekies aangesien dit die enigste vraelys beskikbaar is wat gebaseer is op medinnus en johnson 4 se teoretiese model wat verwerping teenoor aanvaarding asook oortoegeeflikheid en oorbeskerming as 'n houding teenwoordig by die moeder insluit. verder voldoen die vraelys ook aan die statistiese kriteria van betroubaarheid en geldigheid. 5 . prosedure verkryging van proefpersone name en adresse van kinders met genoemde afwykings is verkry van die kliniekhoofde aan die spraaken gehoorkliniek van die universiteit van pretoria, van die spraakkliniek by die h. f. verwoerdhospitaal in pretoria, 'n remedierende onderwyseres in pretoria en 'n chirurg in pretoria. name van moeders met normale kinders is verkry van 'n privaat creche in pretoria en 'n staatsdepartement in pretoria. toepassing . die vraelyste is onder verskillende omstandighede aan die moeders oorhandig en ingevul. sommige moeders het die vraelys ontvang van die terapeut betrokke by hulle kinders se spraakterapie, die vraelyste is by die kliniek ingevul of tuis. sommige van die vraelyste is aan die moeders persoonlik oorhandig en tuis ingevul, ander vraelyste is aan die moeders gepos nadat hulle telefonies gekontak is. alle moeders kon egter nie gekontak word nie; vraelyste is dus slegs uitgepos. die voltooide vraelyste is weer aan die betrokke terapeute oorhandig of aan die ondersoekster self. vraelyste wat uitgepos is, is teruggepos. nasien en berekening van skale elke stelling word bereken in terme van 'n vyfpuntskaal. regs van elke stelling word die skale aangegee nl. ss (stem sterk saam), s (stem saam), ο (onseker), v (verskil) en sv (sterk verskil). die nommerwaardes 5 4 3 2 1 bokant die response is die individuele roupunte vir elke respons. regs van elke stelling is die letters a-ob-ot-v-vd. hierdie letters identifiseer die houdingsskale. ten einde die roupunt te bereken van die houdingskale van bv. verwerping (v) word al die nommerwaardes van die respons ge'identifiseer deur die letter v, op te tel. dieselfde metode word gevolg vir oorbeskerming (ob), oortoegeeflikheid (οτ), aanvaarding (a). hoe hoer die skaaltelling, hoe sterker is die houding. hoe laer die skaaltelling, hoe swakker is die houding. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 118 ronel steenekamp statistiese verwerking die data is verwerk met behulp van die universiteit van pretoria se ibm 370 rekenoutomaat. die "statistical package for the social sciences" (spss) rekenaarprogram is vir die doel gebruik. die volgende subprogramme is betrek: — berekening van die beduidendheid van verskille tussen die groepe (t-toets). — produk-moment-korrelasie. die statistiese formules wat betrekking het op bogenoemde berekenings word deur nie et al 6 volledig bespreek. resultate agt en sewentig vraelyste is terugontvang en verwerk. hiervan was 34 van toepassing op die kontrolegroep, 24 op die minimale-breindisfunksiegroep en 20 op die gesplete verhemeltegroep. (die bevindings word in tabel 2 tot 6 weergegee.) 1 . die aard van die moeder-kind-verhouding dit blyk dat daar 'n statistics beduidende verwerpingsingesteldheid teenwoordig is by die moeders met kinders met minimale-breindisfunksie en 'n spraakafwyking. alhoewel nie statistics beduidend nie, neig hierdie moeders ook om hulle kinders te oorbeskerm. hierdie resultate kom ooreen met die van ander studies. aangesien hierdie kinders uiters baie energie vereis a.g.v. hulle swak eeten slaappatroon, ontydige gehuil, hiperof hipoaktiwiteit, emosionele onstabiliteit en swak aandagspan, kan daar by die moeder 'n vyandigheid ontstaan teenoor die kind wat daartoe lei dat die moeder die kind verwerp. infantiliteit van die kind veroorsaak dat hy afhanklik bly van die moeder wat daartoe lei dat sy hom oorbeskerm. precht7 bevestig hierdie resultate in sy studie "the mother-child interaction in babies with minimal brain damage" (sien tabel 2). daar kan tot die gevolgtrekking gekom word dat daar nie 'n beduidend nie (sien tabel 3). hierdie resultate kom ooreen met die kinders en moeders met gesplete lip en/of verhemeltekinders nie. daar is egter 'n neiging tot oorbeskerming, maar dit is nie statistics beduidend nie (sien tabel 3). hierdie· resultate kom ooreen-met die van g o o d s t e i n . 1 ' 2 goodstein vind ook geen beduidende verskil in die gesindheid van moeders met kinders met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte nie en moeders met normale kinders nie. die neiging tot oorbeskerming by die moeders kan verklaar word deur palmer se semantiese teorie. hy beweer datj 'n semantiese fout ontstaan a.g.v. die "gesond-siek" digotomie. die moeder beskou dus haar kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte as 'n ,,siek" kind en neig om hom te oorbeskerm. ι ι the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) m o e d e r k i n d v e r h o u d i n g 1 1 9 2 . die verband tussen die bepaalde gesinsveranderlikes en die moeder-kind-verhouding d i e v e r b a n d is o o k b e r e k e n t u s s e n d i e h o u d i n g v a n d i e m o e d e r e n d i e b e p a a l d e g e s i n s v e r a n d e r l i k e s v i r d i e d r i e g r o e p e g e s a m e n t l i k . d i t i s g e d o e n v o l g e n s p e a r s o n s e p r o d u k m o m e n t k o r r e l a s i e t e g n i e k ( s i e n t a b e l 4 ) . tabel ii: b e d u i d e n h e i d van verskille in die m o e d e r k i n d v e r h o u d i n g tussen die k o n t r o l e g r o e p e n die m i n i m a l e b r e i n d i s f u n k s i e g r o e p veranderlikes gemiddeld standaard afwyking τ waarde grade van vryheid beduidenheidsvlak oorbeskerming: kontrole mbd 34,47 37,75 6,881 6,589 1,82 56 0,074 verwerping: kontrole mbd 32,05 35,00 4,445 5,927 2,16 56 0,035* oortoegeeflikheid: kontrole mbd 31,88 33,83 5,068 6,844 1,25 56 0,217 aanvaarding: kontrole mbd 36,44 36,37 5,685 6,398 0,04 56 0,967 'beduidend op die 5% vlak. tabel iii: b e d u i d e n h e i d v a n verskille in die m o e d e r k i n d v e r h o u d i n g tussen die k o n t r o l e g r o e p e n die g e s p l e t e lip e n / o f v e r h e m e l t e g r o e p veranderlikes gemiddeld standaard afwyking τ waarde grade van vryheid beduidenheidsvlak oorbeskerming: kontrole gesplete lip 34,47 38,05 6,881 7,097 1,82 52 0,074 verwerping: kontrole gesplete lip 32,05 34,65 4,445 6,556 1,73 52 0,090 oortoegeeflikheid: kontrole gesplete lip 31,88 33,15 5,068 5,153 0,85 52 0 , 3 8 2 aanvaarding: kontrole gesplete lip 36,44 35,55 5,685 14,796 0,59 52 0,559 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 120 ronel steenekamp tabel i v : v e r b a n d tussen b e p a a l d e g e s i n s v e r a n d e r l i k e s e n die m o e d e r k i n d v e r h o u d i n g vir die drie g r o e p e g e s a m e n t l i k ( n = 78). veranderlikes oorbeskerming r verwerping γ oortoegeeflikheid γ aanvaarding r ouderdom van moeder 0,0910 ρ = 0,214 0,0738 ρ = 0,260 0,0491 ρ = 0,335 0,0504 ρ = 0,331 aantal kinders 0,0061 ρ = 0,479 0,0416 ρ = 0,359 0,0295 ρ = 0,899 0,0448 ρ = 0,348 geboorte-orde 0,0429 ρ = 0,354 0,0509 ρ = 0,329 0,0441 ρ = 0,351 0,1143 ρ = 0,159 ouderdom van kind 0,0991 ρ = 0,194 0,2645 ρ = 0,110 0,0439 ρ = 0,351 0,0673 ρ = 0,279 beroep: vader * *0,2885 ρ = 0,005 •0,2141 ρ = 0,030 0,1463 ρ = 0,101 * 0,2103 ρ = 0,032 beroep: moeder 0,0613 ρ = 0,297 * *0,2862 ρ = 0,006 0,1463 ρ = 0,101 0,1155 ρ = 0,157 * 'beduidend op die 1% vlak. •beduidend op die 5% vlak. tabel v : v e r b a n d tussen b e p a a l d e g e s i n s v e r a n d e r l i k e s e n d i e m o e d e r k i n d v e r h o i i d i n g b y d i e m i n i m a l e b r e i n d i s f u n k s i e g o e p ( n = 2 4 ) veranderlikes oorbeskerming γ verwerping γ oortoegeeflikheid ...r.. aanvaarding r ouderdom van moeder 0,1830 ρ = 0,196 0,1792 ρ = 0,201 0,1918 ρ = 0,185 0,2057 ρ = 0,167 aantal kinders 0,1519 ρ = 0,239 0,2598 ρ = 0,110 0,0356 ρ = 0,434 0,1233 ρ = 0,283 geboorte-orde * 0,3620 ρ = 0,041 * 0,3433 ρ = 0,050 0,2580 ρ = 0,112 *0,3975 ρ = 0,027 / 0,0619 ρ = 0,387 ouderdom van kind 0,0686 ρ = 0,375 ; 0,1904 ρ = 0,186 0,1110 ρ = 0,303 *0,3975 ρ = 0,027 / 0,0619 ρ = 0,387 beroep: vader 0,0226 ρ = 0,458 0,3205 ρ' = 0,063 0,0254 ρ = 0,453 0,0524 ρ = 0,404 beroep: moeder 0,2576 ρ = 0,112 ] *0,4326 ρ ' = 0,017 "0,3571 ρ = 0,043 0,2568 ρ = 0,113 'beduidend op die 5% vlak. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) moeder-kind verhouding 121 tabel vi: die verband tussen bepaalde gesinsveranderlikes en die moederkind-verhouding by die gesplete lip en/of verhemelte groep (n = 20) veranderlikes oorbeskerming r verwerping γ oortoegeeflikheid r aanvaarding r ouderdom van moeder 0,2966 ρ = 0,102 0,2110 ρ = 0,186 0,0311 ρ = 0,448 0,2163 ρ = 0,180 aantal kinders 0,3108 ρ = 0,091 0,2340 ρ = 0,160 0,1457 ρ = 0,270 0,0263 ρ = 0,456 geboorte-orde 0,2346 ρ = 0,160 0,0824 ρ = 0,365 0,1048 ρ = 0,330 0,0563 ρ = 0,407 ouderdom van kind 0,3500 ρ = 0,065 *0,3966 ρ = 0,042 0,0360 ρ = 0,440 0,1494 ρ = 0,265 beroep: vader 0,2449 ρ = 0,149 0,1667 ρ = 0,241 0,0174 ρ = 0,471 0,1532 ρ = 0,260 beroep: moeder 0,1244 ρ = 0,301 0,1490 ρ = 0,265 0,1210 ρ = 0,306 0,1546 ρ = 0,258 uit bogenoemde resultate kan afgelei word dat die veranderlikes, ouderdom van die moeder, aantal kinders, geboorte-orde en ouderdom van die kind geen invloed het op die houding van die moeder nie. dit is egter in teenstelling met ander literatuur wat hierdie veranderlikes as belangrik beskou in die ontwikkeling van die moeder-kind-verhouding. 'n positiewe verband bestaan egter tussen die beroep van die vader en oorbeskerming en verwerping (nie-aanvaardingsdimensies) by die moeder. daar bestaan 'n negatiewe verband tussen die beroep van die vader en aanvaarding deur die moeder van haar kind. hier kan dus afgelei word dat daar 'n groter ingesteldheid tot nieaanvaarding bestaan by moeders wat getroud is met mans in die sogenoemde laervlakberoepe. 'n positiewe verband is ook gevind t.o.v. die beroep van die moeder en volgens hierdie bevindings blyk dit of moeders wat huisvrouens is, in 'n groter mate geneig is om hulle kinders te verwerp as werkende moeders. daar is 'n negatiewe verband tussen die geboorte-orde van die kind en oorbeskerming sowel as verwerping by kinders met minimalebrein-disfunksie (sien tabel 5). dit wil se hoe laer die kind met minimale-brein-disfunksie in die geboorte-orde voorkom, hoe minder word hy oorbeskerm of verwerp deur die moeder. hierdie neiging word bevestig deur 'n positiewe verband tussen die geboorte-orde van die kind en aanvaarding van die kind. dit toon aan dat hoe laer die kind in die geboorte-orde voorkom, hoe meer die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 122 ronel steenekamp word hy deur die moeder aanvaar. eersgeborenes met minimalebrein-disfunksie sal dus meer oorbeskerm en verwerp word en minder aanvaar word as latere kinders. precht7 beweer dat moeders met kinders wat minimale-breindisfunksie toon, die simptome van hulle kinders as deel van die kind se temperament en persoonlikheid aanvaar. die kind word dus aanvaar, maar sy gedrag stel die moeder nie tevrede nie. die moeder raak fisies uitgeput en gei'rriteerd a.g.v. hierdie gedrag van die kind, wat daartoe lei dat sy die kind verwerp. die infantiliteit van die kind gee aanleiding tot die moeder se oorbeskerming. medinnus en johnson 4 beweer dat moeders 'n baie sterker band met hulle eerste kinders opbou en dus baie meer betrokke is by die gebeure rondom die kind as latere kinders. fisieseof gedragsafwykings by eersgeborenes sal dus 'n baie groter invloed he op die moeder se houding teenoor die kind. 'n positiewe verband is ook gevind t.o.v. die beroep van die moeder en 'n nie-aanvaardingsingesteldheid. dit blyk dat moeders wat huisvrouens is, in 'η groter mate geneig is om hulle kinders te verwerp, wat moontlik toegeskryf kan word aan die feit dat sy vir baie langer periodes blootgestel is aan die kind met minimale-brein-disfunksie se gedrag. sy is ook geneig om oortoegeeflik te wees teenoor die kind. hierdie neiging kan toegeskryf word aan die feit dat daar aan die moeders gese word dat die kind nie beheer oor sy gedrag het nie en dat sy dit nie kan verander nie. sy gee dus makliker toe aan sy versoeke en ontoelaatbare gedrag. die ouderdom van die kind toon wel 'n beduidende verband met verwerping by kinders met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte (sien tabel 6). hoe ouer die kind met 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte is, hoe groter is die verwerping. hierdie resultate dui daarop dat die moeder, namate die kind ouer word, hom meer verwerp. dit word bevestig deur goodstein.1 goodstein verklaar dit soos volg: . . . the effects on parental adjustment of having a child with this type of physical handicap in the home are not immediately evidenced, but rather develop with time. baie van die probleme by die kinders, soos spraakprobleme, ortodontiese probleme, chirurgiesof ander sekondere probleme is nie aan die moeder bekend by geboorte nie, maar word belangriker namate die kind ouer word. gepaardgaande emosionele en sosiale afwykings wat manifesteer in swak skoolprestasie en leerprobleme kan die moeder se verwerpingsingesteldheid versterk. 1 / ] gevolgtrekkings , daar kan nie tot die gevolgtrekking gekom word dat daar 'n beduidende verskil in die moeder-kind-verhouding van moeders met minimale-brein-disfunksie of gesplete llip en/of verhemelte kindes teenoor moeders met normale kinders is nie, aangesien bogenoemde resultate nie beduidend genoeg is nie. dit kan die gevolg wees van die the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) moeder-kind verhouding 123 feit dat die groepe nie groot genoeg was nie en 'n normale verspreiding nie voorgekom het nie. die moontlikheid bestaan ook dat die "mother-child-relationshipevaluation"-vraelys nie sensitief genoeg is om verskille aan te toon in die houding van moeders met kinders met spesifieke abnormaliteite teenoor moeders met normale kinders nie. goodstein1 en moll & darley5 het ook gevind dat 'n enkele vraelys of toets nie voldoende is om ingesteldhede by ouers met kinders met spesifieke abnormaliteite teenoor ouers met normale kinders waar te neem nie. daar kan egter wel afgelei word dat moeders met kinders wat minimale-brein-disfunksie toon, geneig is om hierdie kinders te verwerp. die verwerpingsingesteldheid van die moeders kan 'n bydraende faktor wees in die spraakafwykings wat by die kinders voorkom. as gevolg van die moeder se verwerping, is die kind voortdurend gefrustreerd, maar hy mag nie uiting gee aan die gevolge daarvan nie. die kind moet hierdie aggressiewe gevoelens onderdruk en leer dus dat spraak 'n suksesvolle wapen is. die feit dat daar geen statisties beduidende verskil gevind is in die houding van die moeders met gesplete lip en/of verhemelte kinders nie, kan waarskynlik toegeskryf word aan die feit dat die moeders reeds die afgelope aantal jare hulp ontvang vanuit 'n multidissiplinere raamwerk. die nulhipotese word dus aanvaar nl. dat daar geen beduidende verskil tussen die moeder-kind-verhouding van die onderskeie ondersoekgroepe en die kontrolegroep is nie. die werkhipotese ( h : en h 2 ) word dus verwerp nl. dat die moeder-kind-verhouding betekenisvol swakker of beter is by die onderskeie ondersoekgroepe as die van die kontrole groep. daar is ook gevind dat sommige gesinsveranderlikes wel 'n invloed kan he op die moeder-kind-verhouding veral by moeders met kinders wat 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte het of minimale-brein-disfunksie. aanbevelings dit word aanbeveel dat, indien daar in verdere navorsing in die verband belangstel word, 'n battery toetse uitgevoer word eerder as 'n enkele toets, aangesien 'n enkele toets slegs een fasset toets en daar wel verskille op ander vlakke kan voorkom. in verdere navorsing behoort daar veral klem gele te word op die moeder-kind-verhouding by kinders met minimale-brein-disfunksie, aangesien hierdie studie wel 'n verwerpingsneiging uitgewys het by moeders met kinders wat minimale-brein-disfunksie toon. slot in die algemeen kan tot die gevolgtrekking gekom word dat die evaluering van die moeder-kind-verhouding met behulp van die "mother-child-relationship-evaluation"-vraelys van min waarde was die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 124 ronel steenekamp in 'n poging om die moeders van kinders met minimale-breindisfunksie of 'n gesplete lip en/of verhemelte en hulle verhouding met die kind beter te verstaan. dit het egter wel aan die lig gebring dat moeders met kinders wat minimale-brein-disfunksie het, nie hulle kinders bevredigend aanvaar nie. hierdie resultate beklemtoon weereens die belang van 'n multidimensionele benadering in behandeling van hierdie kinders. verwysings 1. goodstein, l. d. (1960a): personality test differences in parents of children with cleft palates. j. speech. hear. res. 3. p. 39-43. 2. goodstein, l. d. (1960b): mmpi differences between parents of children with cleft palates and parents of physically normal children. j. speech. hear. res. 3. p. 31-37. 3. klaus, μ. h . & kennel, j. h. (1976): maternal infant bonding c. v. hosby st. louis. 4. meddinus, g. r. & johnson, r. c. (1969): child and adolescent psychology: behaviour and development. wiley & sons. ny. 5. moll, k. l. & darley, f. l. (1960): attitudes of mothers of articulatory impaired and speech retarded children. j. speech. hear. dis. 25. pp. 377-384. 6. nie, ν. h. (1975): statistical package for the social sciences. mcgraw hill. · 7. precht, m. f. r. (1963): the mother child interaction in babies with minimal brain damage (a follow-up study). in determinants of infant behaviour vol. 2. foss (ed). butler and tanner ltd. g. b . 8. roth, m. r. (1961): the mother child relationship evaluation western psychological services. california. 9. symonds, p. (1949): dynamics of parent child relationships. bureau of publications ν. y. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) philips hearing aid services a division of s.a. philips (pty) ltd. hearing aids amplaid audiometers group teaching systems p h i l i p s hearing aid services head office 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street, p.o. box 3069, johannesburg. p h i l i p s fib2399 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) i n t r o d u c i n g ... t y m p a n o s c o p e t h e i m p e d a n c e m e t e r t h a t u s e s a n o t o s c o p e a s t h e p r o b e ... b y madsen electronics 1 s t in impedance • automatic • fast • portable • printout • diagnostic • screening • otoscopy • tympanometry — from +200 to -300 mm hjo • reilex — ipsilaieral in real lime contact kan southcott or richard anderson at amtronix (pty) ltd p.o. box 630 bedtordview 2008 phone 61s-7647/8/9 performs all tests automatically — complete lest lakes only 10 seconds each ear — weighs only 15 lbs. (7 kg) — ibm size card lot r?asv nlirig — results correlate with marisra'i 1 lical instrument — pass •' lail para m el era adjustable — pressure finely control led r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e acquisition of some dimensional adjectives by both n o r m a l a n d l a n g u a g e i m p a i r e d children susan gail wright, b . a . ( s p . & h . therapy) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) psychological and guidance services, transvaal education department, johannesburg north-west, johannesburg. summary the main aim of this study was to assess various predictions made by h. and e. clark with respect to the acquisition of certain dimensional adjectives. in addition, the performance of children with impaired language skills was compared with that of children with normally developing language. eighteen subjects in the age range 3,3 to 4 years were divided into two groups; those with adequate language (c group) and those with impaired language (e group). the dimensional adjective pairs of "length", "tallness" and "width" were investigated on comprehension tasks of increasing dimensionality. a qualitative analysis of the data, for both c and ε groups, revealed findings supporting the predictions concerning the order of dimensional adjective acquisition in terms of semantic complexity, the acquisition of the unmarked pair member before the marked member, and the acquisition of the concept of polarity before dimensionality. a quantitative analysis of the data revealed significant differences between the c and ε groups on a few tasks only. implications for the researcher and speech therapist are considered. opsomming die hoofdoel van hierdie studie was om die verskillende voorspellings van h. clark en e. clark, met betrekking tot die verwering van sekere dimensionele byvoeglike naamwoorde, te ondersoek. daarbenewens is die prestasie van kinders met ontoereikende taalvermoens vergelyk met di6 van kinders met normaalontwikkelende taal. agtien proefpersone, vanaf 3,3 tot 4 jaar, was verdeel in twee groepe; di6 met voldoende taal (groep c) en di6 met ontoereikende taal (groep e). die pare dimensionele byvoeglike naamwoorde van "lengte" en "breedte" is ondersoek m.v. begripstoetse van toenemende dimensionaliteit. 'n kwalitatiewe analise van die gegewens vir albei groepe, het voorspellings aangaande die verwerings volgorde van dimensionele byvoeglike naamwoorde, gebaseer op semantiese ingewikkeldheid, ondersteun — naamlik, die verwerwirig van die ongemerkte deel van die paar voor die gemerkte lid en van die begrip van polariteit v66r dimensionaliteit. 'n kwantitatiewe analise van die gegewens het betekenisvolle verskille tussen groep c en ε op slegs 'n paar take aangedui. die implikasies vir die navorser en spraakterapeut is bespreek. the relationship between cognition and language has long been a topic of discussion and presents a controversial area of study. the importance of a conceptual basis for language development is particularly evident when reviewing recent literature concerning the early acquisition of language.4 within this field, attempts have been made to investigate the nature of the acquisition of dimensional adjectives. 2 ' 1 3 as before, results have revealed varying degrees of disagreement and an integrated overview of this subdivision has yet to be achieved. the present study was undertaken with a view to researching various proposals put forward by the existing theories. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 128 susan wright "most of language is composed of relational t e r m s " 1 1 of which dimensional adjectives form a sub-set. h . clark,7· 8 · 9 in researching the progression and nature of the acquisition of spatial and temporal expressions, has provided a theoretical framework which is widely discussed in the literature.1 2· 1 7 his hypotheses generally appear to be considered noteworthy and comprehensive, although perhaps not fully explanatory. h. clark9 bases the evidence for this thesis on the "strong correspondence between the properties of spatial terms and the properties of man's innate perceptual apparatus". he postulates the existence of a perceptual space, or p-space, and a linguistic space, or l-space; thus supporting the notion of a connection between cognition and language. the properties of l-space are predicted to be identical to those of p-space; thus forming his correlation hypothesis.9 biological and physical environments place constraints on the way in which objects may be described in space.9 these constraints are determined by p-space properties, which make varying demands on man's perceptions. an area which is easily perceptible may be considered a "positive" perceptual direction as opposed to a "negative" one. similarly, linguistic forms may differ in complexity;, the more complex term is "marked" with respect to the less complex term. the "positive" or "unmarked" term may be comprehended more easily than the "negative" or "marked" t e r m . 9 the complexity hypothesis is inherent in this proposal. dimensional adjectives are divided into adjective pairs defining the dimensions of size, length, distance, tallness, height, depth, width, breadth, and thickness.8 these pairs comprise big-small, long-short, far -near, tall-short, high-low, deep-shallow, wide-narrow, broadnarrow, and thick-thin. in terms of the "markedness" theory,9 the first member of each pair is "unmarked" and the second "marked". this theory is further supported by e. clark5· 6 in her advancement of the semantic feature hypothesis (sfh). sfh predicts that the child does not randomly decide which of the two meanings to attribute to the antonym pair, but rather operates systematically by selecting the member of the pair which is linguistically simpler or "unmarked" before he acquires the "marked" term. to date, research findings concerning "markedness" have been contradictory.2· u · 1 3 the notion of "markedness" is prominent in another aspect of h. clark's theory concerning the sequence of acquisition/of the dimensional adjective pairs. he proposes that the pairs differ in their conditions of application according to the nature of their dimensionality.9 the fewer dimensions an adjective presupposes, the less complex the adjective will be, and therefore the less "marked". adjective pairs involving only one dimension will be acquired before those involving two or three dimensions.9 other authorities have found support for this prediction.1· 3 h. clark8 therefore proposes that the intrinsic properties of the the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acquisition of dimensional adjectives 129 antonym pairs could affect their sequence of acquisition. he suggests that the child first uses the terms in a nominal, non-comparative sense only. here the appropriate dimension is indicated disregarding polarity; for example, both "long" and "short" mean "having length". the child gradually begins to distinguish between the terms and to use them comparatively. bartlett2 has failed to find support for these predictions concerning polarity. her data suggests that the concept of polarity is acquired before the concept of dimensionality. as soon as stimuli varied along a number of dimensions, judgements relating to one polar feature were more difficult. little research has been carried out to investigate this particular prediction. in conclusion, the correlation and complexity hypotheses, proposed by h. clark8' 9 make various predictions concerning the acquisition of dimensional adjectives. research findings have supported or rejected these predictions to varying degrees. literature concerning the comprehension of dimensional adjectives in children with impaired language skills is scarce. although leonard, bolders and miller 1 4 believe that these children tend to be delayed in terms of semantic relations, many questions remain unanswered. does the pattern of dimensional adjective acquisition in these children match that of children with adequate language skills? is there a difference in performance, on dimensional adjective comprehension tasks, between children with receptive and expressive language impairment? further investigation is therefore indicated. m e t h o d this study aimed to assess various predictions of h. clark's theories and the sfh proposed by e. clark, with respect to the acquisition of certain dimensional adjectives. the comprehension of these adjectives by children with delayed language skills was compared to that of children with normally developing language skills. h y p o t h e s e s 1. the acquisition of dimensional adjectives, in ss with normal language skills, follows a particular developmental sequence determined by the semantic complexity of the terms:— "long-short" to "tall-short" to "wide-narrow". 2. the unmarked member of a dimensional pair is acquired before the marked member of the pair, in ss with normal language skills. 3. the concept of polarity is acquired before the concept of dimensionality, in ss with normal language skills. 4. on examination of h 1 ; h 2 and h 3 in relation to language impaired ss, a difference exists in the acquisition of dimensional adjectives by language impaired ss. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 130 susan wright 5. a difference exists between the performance of ss with receptive and expressive language impairments with regard to the acquisition of various dimensional adjectives. s u b j e c t s eight children with adequate language skills and eight children with delayed language skills, ranging in age from 3,3 to 4 years, were selected. the mean age of both the c and ε ss was 3,6 years. subjects were selected according to certain criteria:— 1. within an age range of 3,3 to 4 years, children with normally developing language are reported to be able to differentiate between the members of the dimensional adjective pairs under investigation.13 evidence that the acquisition of these adjectives continues after 4 years,2 suggests that the chosen age range will provide an opportunity to study the progression in development of the structures at this stage. 2. both c and ε groups comprised seven male ss and one female s as this reflected the ratio of males to females attending the speech clinics visited. this incidence is supported in the literature. 1 5 3. the ε ss were diagnosed by a speech therapist as having delayed language skills (receptive or expressive) for their age. they had all been attending speech therapy for at least six months. the c ss were each considered, by mothers and nursery school teachers, to have adequate language skills for their age. in addition, an objective measure was used to assess language skills. the verbal comprehension scale a section of the reynell developmental language scale 1 6 was administered to all ss. this test acted as a screening device during the selection of c ss. in the ε group, ss with a receptive language delay were separated from those with a purely expressive delay. 4. all ss were required to have attended a nursery school for a minimum of six months in order to control for the influence of schooling. each ε s was paired with a c s of same sex and a similar age from the same nursery school to control further for possible differences in the schooling environments. 5. all ss were required to come from middle to upper-middle class homes in an attempt to ensure similar environmental stimulation. 6. all ss were judged as being of normal intelligence and to have no primary behavioural or emotional involvement. 7. ss were required to have adequate hearing. t e s t m a t e r i a l s : the ss' levels of comprehension of various dimensional adjectives was the purpose of this study and a pertinent task was thus devised. three pairs of dimensional adjectives were chosen for this study, each pair representing one of the three dimensions of length, tallness and width. the properties of these adjectives are displayed in table i. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acquisition of dimensional adjectives 131 t a b l e i : summary of dimensional adjective properties proposed by h . clark.9 "long-short" "tall-short" "wide-narrow" extent or number of point of position dimensions verticality reference + extent 1 ego (primary) + extent 3 • ground level (primary) + extent 2 secondary edge although the pair implying width involves only two dimensions, it will be more cognitively and semantically complex than that of tallness, involving three dimensions, since it refers to the secondary feature of a reference object. the perception of a secondary feature, rather than that of a primary feature, is necessarily more complex. these particular dimensional adjective pairs were therefore chosen because of their varying complexities. the unmarked members of the dimensional pairs are, according to h. clark9 "long", "tall" and "wide". the marked pair members are "short", "short" and "narrow" respectively. n a t u r e o f t h e s t i m u l i a n d t h e e q u i p m e n t u s e d the stimuli consisted of object pairs which were varied systematically along certain dimensions according to the relevant dimensional adjective pair and the complexity of the task required. 1. to assess the acquisition of features of polarity, apart from the acquisition of dimensionality, only the relevant dimension was varied, that is the objects differed along only a single dimension; one pair differed in length (strings of beads), one in tallness (cardboard human figures) and one in width (cardboard strips). 2. to assess the acquisition of features of dimensionality it was necessary to use objects which varied along at least two dimensions to determine if the child was able to extract the relevant dimension from the properties of the stimulus.2 the dimensions were varied systematically so as to determine the effect of different dimensional features on the acquisition of the relevant dimension. doubledimensional stimuli consisted of a combination of two adjective pairs. multi-dimensional stimuli consisted of various combinations of all the adjective pairs simultaneously. table ii presents the materials used with respect to the relevant dimensions. t e s t p r o c e d u r e each s was tested individually in a quiet room with limited distractions. testing, with a few exceptions, was carried out in the nursery school as this had the advantage of familiarity of environment of the child. each s was tested during a single session lasting approximately forty minutes. inconsistencies found when examining die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 132 susan wright table ii: description of doubleand multi-dimensional stimuli stimulus pairs task no. l e n g t h (+primary) t a l l n e s s (+primary) w i d t h (+secondary) long short tall short wide narrow ό _j cardboard flags two strips of paper 1 2 3 4 long short short long long short short long ι— m ζ a —1 : r ζ ο c/3 ζ u_l s ο i i i cardboard dogs cardboard trees 1 2 3 4 tall short short tall tall short short tall —1 > 1— 1— ζ m co co cd ^ ο ο two strips of paper cardboard houses 1 2 3 4 wide narrow narrow wide wide narrow narrow wide s σ —i :r s _j green blocks 1 2 3 4 long long short short short short long long long short long short short long short long 1— m ζ a —1 x ζ ο c/3 ζ u_l ξ ο •ή pink blocks 1 2 3 4 tall tall short short short short tall tall tall short tall short short tall short tall 2 ' 1— f— ζ m od oo ξ blue blocks 1 2 3 4 wide wide narrow narrow narrow narrow wide wide wide narrow wide narrow narrow wide narrow wide s σ —i x d d ; for example, task 1 for the adjective pair "long-short" involved the contrasting of two flags of the dimensions:— (1) "long-tall" (2) "short-short". m-d; for example, task 1 for the adjective pair "long-short' involved the contrasting of two blocks of the dimensions: (1) long/tall/wide (2) short/short/narrow. various research results concerning the field of acquisition of dimensional adjectives could be attributed to their varying methodologies.2' 1 3 the task type of this study was based on that used by bartlett2 since her method of eliciting responses indicating comprehension'appears to exclude variables such as cognitive complexity. a pair of objects was placed in front of the s who was asked to "give me the (object)", where " " was one of the dimensional adjectives and (object) was the name of the stimulus, for example "give me the long dog". two pre-test tasks ensured the s's understanding of the directions. each object pair was presented twice; once when the unmarked adjective of the pair was asked for and once when the marked adjective was required. objects were presented to each s in random order, within the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acquisition of dimensional adjectives 133 the adjective pair categories, except for the requirement that no object pair was presented twice in succession. scoring consisted of correct/incorrect marking to promote statistical analysis. information to aid in a qualitative analysis was also recorded. results two types of analyses were applied to the data. a quantitative analysis, the mann-whitney u-test for independent samples, 1 0 determined whether a statistically significant difference existed between the various measures obtained for the c and ε groups. significance at a 0,05 level for a one-tailed test was determined. a qualitative analysis in the form of descriptive evaluation of the findings obtained by both groups highlighted trends in the findings. the results are discussed within a framework provided by the hypotheses. overall test results are presented in table iii. the results of the statistical test are presented in table iv. table iii: number and percentag^of correct responses obtained by ε and c subjects α length tallness width ο th o long short ( + ) ( ) tall short (+) ( ) wide narrow ( + ) ( ) s in g l e ε c 7/87,5% 6/75% 13/81% 8/100% 7/87,5% 15/94% 4/50% 5/62,5% 9/56% 7/87,5% 5/62,5% 12/75% 6/75% 2/25% 8/50% 5/62,5% 5/62,5% 10/62,5% d o u b l e ε c 24/75% 18/56% 42/66% 27/84% 24/75% 51/80% 13/41% 15/47% 28/44% 27/84% 20/62,5% 47/73% 11/34% 5/16% 16/25% 21/66% 15/47% 36/56% m u l t i ε c 27/84% 13/41% 40/62.5% 32/100% 26/81% 58/91% 16/50% 13/41% 29,45% 22/69% 20/62,5% 42/66% 9/28% 3/9% 12/19% 14/44% 13/41% 27/42% ( + ) = unmarked term, ( ) = marked term. 1. sequence of acquisition of dimensional adjective pairs the obtained percentages indicate that there is a distinct sequence of acquisition in the comprehension of the three dimensional adjective pairs across all tasks for single-, doubleand multi-dimensional stimuli; that is, length to tallness to width. h j may therefore be accepted. the ε group also demonstrates a similar sequence in the acquisition of the dimensional terms. this sequence is not particularly well-defined for the single-dimensional terms. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 134 susan wright table iv: results of the mann-whitney u-test comparing c and ε group performance category dimension /adjective pair u implications (e ss) sequence of acquisition of dimensional adjective pairs single length tallness width double length tallness width multi length tallness width 36,5 | not significantly 40,5 v different or 38 j delayed. sequence of acquisition of dimensional adjective pairs single length tallness width double length tallness width multi length tallness width 46,5 not delayed. *49 1 delayed in adjective *55,5 j acquisition. sequence of acquisition of dimensional adjective pairs single length tallness width double length tallness width multi length tallness width *58,5 delayed. 41,5 not delayed. *52 delayed. order of acquisition of adjective pair members single length + tallness + width + double length + tallness + width + multi length + tallness + width + 36 η 36 44 32 28 j not significantly >• different or delayed. order of acquisition of adjective pair members single length + tallness + width + double length + tallness + width + multi length + tallness + width + 34 45 48,5 40,5 48,5 *53,5 ^ not significantly ί delayed. delayed for marked term. order of acquisition of adjective pair members single length + tallness + width + double length + tallness + width + multi length + tallness + width + *52 1 delayed for marked and *54,5 j unmarked term. , \ not significantly s j delayed. *51,5 delayed for marked term. sequence of acquisition of polarity and dimensionality length single double multi tallness single double multi width single double | multi 36.5 ί not significantly 45.6 j delayed on these tasks. *58,5 delayed when task complex sequence of acquisition of polarity and dimensionality length single double multi tallness single double multi width single double | multi 40,5 not delayed. *49 delayed on doublelevel. 41,5 not delayed. sequence of acquisition of polarity and dimensionality length single double multi tallness single double multi width single double | multi 38 "i not delayed. / "55,5 > delayed in the acqiiisi'52 j tion of dimensionality. ( + ) = unmarked term, ( ) = marked term ; * indicates a statistically significant result with u 3= 49. on comparing the c and ess on their; task performance ess do not show any uniform significant delay across all the comprehension tasks of the adjective pairs, as expected. significant differences were obtained on double-dimensional tasks of tallness and width, and on the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acquisition of dimensional adjectives 135 multi-dimensional tasks of length and width indicating a delay of the ε group in these areas. h 4 , in relation to h 1 ; may therefore only be partly accepted. 2. order of acquisition of adjective pair members many of the findings relating to the css' performance, indicate only a slight discrepancy between the acquisition of unmarked and marked terms (in favour of the unmarked term). only width reveals a more decisive difference between the acquisition of the pair members. however, the writer feels that h 2 may be accepted for reasons considered in the discussion. findings for the ess generally reveal a superiority in the comprehension of the unmarked terms. the only statistically significant difference, between the c and ess, in the acquisition of unmarked and marked terms exists on the multidimensional task involving length. the acquisition of the marked term of width for doubleand multi-dimensional tasks was also significantly different. h 4 , in relation to h 2 , may only be partly accepted. 3. sequence of acquisition of polarity and dimensionality here, the ss' performance on single-dimensional adjective tasks (representing polarity), and doubleand multi-dimensional adjective tasks (both representing a different degree of dimensionality) were considered. findings, for the css, involving tallness and width support h 3 since task difficulty increases from singleto doubleto multidimensional tasks. the ess' performance suggests a similar progression of difficulty for all adjective pairs. the ess do not show any general significant difference from the css across dimensional tasks involving length or tallness. significant differences were found to exist on measures of width. although, both c and ess appear to experience increasing difficulty with increasing dimensionality, the ess demonstrate a significant delay in the acquisition of width, especially for doubleand multi-dimensional tasks. h 4 , in relation to h 3 , may therefore only be accepted for the dimensional adjective pair of width. 4. ess' performance in terms of the presence of a receptive or depressive language delay the ε group was divided equally into ss with receptive and purely expressive language impairment by the language pre-test. 1 6 these two sub-groups were compared on their comprehension of the adjective pairs under all the different task conditions: on application of the statistical test, the groups do not show any significant differences with regard to the acquisition of various dimensional adjectives. h 5 is therefore rejected. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) susan wright discussion sequence of acquisition of dimensional adjective pairs the results obtained by both c and ess did indicate a progression in acquisition from length to tallness to width across all dimensional tasks. this finding therefore supports the trend in the literature.2 , 9 in trying to explain why some adjective pairs should be acquired later than others, it appears to be reasonable to assume that these pairs involve a greater semantic complexity and are thus more difficult to learn. if cognition and language are linked, as suggested in the literature, it is possible that semantic complexity would imply cognitive complexity. these more complex terms would then be acquired later on the developmental scale at a stage when the child's innate cognitive processes are more evolved. a comparison between the abilities of c and ess does not produce such conclusive results. ess appear to be performing on a similar level as css on tasks involving singledimensions. if doubleand multi-dimensional tasks are more complex than single-dimensional tasks,9 this finding could indicate that ess are operating at a less complex level and therefore fail to perform as adequately as css when more demanding tasks are introduced.. if this reasoning is correct, ess may be assumed to be significantly delayed in dimensional adjective acquisition due to their inability to cope as efficiently as css, with the adjective pairs under varying task conditions. these findings would therefore support predictions made by leonard et a l . 1 4 order of acquisition of adjective pair members both c and ess demonstrate superior comprehension of the unmarked member, as opposed to the marked member, of the dimensional adjective pairs. for css, however, there is frequently only a slight discrepancy between the acquisition of unmarked and marked terms across all the dimensional tasks. this could be explained by the relative stability of the adjective pairs in the ss' repertoires; a decisive discrepancy would only be evident during the initial stages of adjective pair acquisition.8 it is evident that findings for unmarked and marked terms become more discrepant when the components of width are involved in the various combinations. this finding could be related to h j suggesting that the adjective pair width is the last acquired in the sequence and is therefore still particularly unstable in the css' repertoires. findings for the ess could also be related to h j ; ess are still in the process of acquiring all the dimensional adjectives. preference for the unmarked term would thus be more decisive at this stage of incomplete acquisition and is apparent during all dimensional tasks of varying complexities. a comparison of the abilities of c and ess reveals only isolated occurrences of a significant difference appearing to highlight a trend in the findings only in that they are evident in the more complex doublethe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acquisition of dimensional adjectives 137 and multi-dimensional tasks. this could be related to h 4 , involving h 1 ; where a significant difference between ε and css was found on these tasks. reduced stability of the pairs in the ess repertoires, as compared to the css, could explain the delay in the ess discrimination between these unmarked and marked terms. these results only partly support predictions made by leonard et a l . 1 4 sequence of acquisition of polarity and dimensionality css demonstrated the acquisition of the concept of polarity prior to that of dimensionality for tallness and width. the absence of increasing difficulty with increasing dimensionality for length could be explained by the relative stability of this term in the css' repertoires (as suggested by h i ) . these findings fail to support the general trend in the literature stating that the comprehension of dimensional features is acquired before that of polarity features.9 they support contradictory research findings obtained by bartlett. 2 results for the ess suggest increasing difficulty with increasing dimensionality for all adjective pairs. it appears that, in this study, bartlett's prediction2 can be extended to implicate language impaired ss. the finding of a significant delay only in the acquisition of width, especially for doubleand multi-dimensional tasks, on comparing the abilities of c and ess could be explained in terms of h 4 , in relation to h j . width, which is the most semantically complex of the adjective pairs studied,5 was found to be significantly delayed for the ess, particularly on the more complex tasks. it is therefore logical that ess should differ significantly from css in this area whereas the difference is not as marked for the other less complex adjective pairs. although both groups show a trend towards the acquisition of polarity before dimensionality, ess are significantly delayed in their acquisition of both of these features. predictions made by leonard et a l 1 4 are only partly accepted since the ess were not significantly delayed in all aspects of this semantic area. conclusions the hypotheses based on predictions made by h. clark8' 9 and sfh, 5 ' that is h i and h 2 , are supported by the data. the prediction could also, according to these findings, be extended to implicate ss with language impairment. h 3 , based on bartlett's p r o p o s a l 2 is accepted thus implying the rejection of the sfh prediction. the findings indicate that the proposal could also describe the performance of language impaired ss. h 4 , in relation to h 1 ; h 2 and h 3 , may be partly accepted indicating significant differences between c and ess' performance in isolated areas only. h 5 is rejected implying similar performance by the ess irrespective of the type of language impairment. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 138 susan wright implications further research in this area is obviously necessary before broad conclusions may be drawn. research could tend towards the establishment of norms for the acquisition of dimensional adjectives, the investigation of the acquisition of other dimensional adjectives, the expression of these adjectives, or the analysis of comparatives in terms of deductive reasoning.7 in devising a therapy programme it appears to be important to consider the normal sequence of development. this study highlighted certain areas: 1. teaching should progress from less complex to more complex terms. 2. the unmarked member of the pair should be taught before the marked member, since this appears to be a less complex term. 3. the terms should be taught in terms of polarity features before further dimensionality is introduced. tasks should be graded in terms of dimensionality; that is, singleto doubleto multidimensional tasks. dimensional adjectives influence the child's perception of his world and could thus affect corresponding areas of language and perception. acknowledgements the writer thanks: mrs. f. schmaman of the department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, for her supervision. references 1. anglin, j. m. (1970): the growth of word meaning. m.i.t. press, london. 2. bartlett, e. j. (1976): sizing things up; the acquisition of the meaning of dimensional adjectives. j. child lang., 3, 205-219. 3. bierwisch, m. (1970): on classifying semantic features. in bierwisch, m. and heidolph, κ. e. (eds.), progress in linguistics. mouton & co., the hague, paris. 4. brown, r. (1976): a first language the early stages. (2nd edition). penguin books ltd., england. 5. clark, ε. v. (1973): what's in a word? on the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. in moore, τ. e. (ed.), cognitive development and the acquisition of language. academic press inc., new york. 6. clark, ε. v. (1974): some aspects of the conceptual basis for first language acquisition. in schiefelbusch, r. l. and lloyd, l. l. (eds.), language perspectives —' acquisition, retardation and intervention. university park press; baltimore. 7. clark, η. h . (1969): linguistic processes in deductive reasoning. psych. review, 76, 4, 387-404. , the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acquisition of dimensional adjectives 139 8. clark, η. h . (1970): the primitive nature of children's relational concepts. in hayes, j. (ed.), cognition and the development of language. wiley, new york. 9. clark, η. h . (1973): space, time, semantics and the child. in moore, τ. e. (ed.), cognitive development and the acquisition of language. academic press inc., new york. 10. connolly, t. g. and sluckin, w. (1971): an introduction to statistics for the social sciences. (3rd edition). the macmillan press ltd., london. 11. donaldson, m. and wales, r. (1970): on the acquisition of some relational terms. in hayes, j. (ed.), cognition and the development of language. wiley, new york. 12. friedman, w. j. and seely, p. b. (1967): the child's acquisition of spatial and temporal word meanings. child develop., 47, 4 , 1103-1108. 13. klatzky, r. l., clark, ε. v. and macken, m. (1973): asymmetries in the acquisition of polar adjectives: linguistic or conceptual? j. experimental child psych., 16, 32-46. 14. leonard, l. b., bolders, j. g. and miller, j. a. (1976): a n examination of the semantic relation reflected in the language usage of normal and language disordered children. j. speech hear. res., 19, 371-392. 15. menyuk, p. (1971): the acquisition and development of language. prentice-hall inc., englewood cliffs, new jersey. 16. reynell, j. (1969): reynell developmental language scale. n . f . e . r . publishing co. ltd., england. 17. townsend, d. j. (1976): do children interpret "marked" comparative adjectives as their opposites?. j. child lang., 3, 385-396. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) professional resources for communications specialists r e a d i n g a n d l a n g u a g e a r t s l a n g u a g e d e v e l o p m e n t a s s e s s m e n t m a t e r i a l s b a s i c s k i l l s ο » for information or free catalogues call read ( p t y ) l t d . / ( e d m s ) b p k . t e l : johannesburg 3 9 6 3 7 8 · pretoria 4 4 4 2 4 2 d u r b a n 6 6 6 6 9 · c a p e t o w n 2 2 0 9 4 7 pietermaritzburg 5 8 0 7 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 23 the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw department of communication pathology, university of pretoria abstract in south africa, the current movement towards the inclusion of children with disabilities, including children with hearing loss, is likely to have far-reaching consequences for both teachers and learners. undoubtedly, needs will arise from teachers during the transition, especially in the areas pertaining to the audiological and educational management of children with hearing loss. therefore, a descriptive research design was developed comprising of a questionnaire survey followed by focus group interviews to determine teachers' needs. the questionnaire survey explored the needs of 664 teachers while focus group interviews were conducted with 19 teachers of children with hearing loss. teachers were mostly from special schools as only a very small number of children are educated outside these establishments. findings revealed that, although participants realised the importance of various aspects of development of the child with hearing loss, they generally did not realise the importance of receiving support from an educational audiologist. key words: children with hearing loss, educational audiologist, educational audiology, inclusion, needs of teachers, teacher support. introduction "...because of the invisible nature of the (hearing) impairment, and the general lack of understanding regarding the full impact of hearing impairment upon learning, there is always a need for individuals to work for the child, to ensure that his or her needs as a learner with hearing impairment are not marginalized or overlooked." (english, 1995 pl2). the education of all learners in south africa, including children with hearing loss, has undergone profound changes since the end of the apartheid era in 1994. the educational system changed from a racially segregated system to a non-racial inclusive system. prior to 1994, specialised education was characterised by the following (education white paper no. 6, 2001): • education and support were predominantly provided for / a small percentage of learners with disabilities within / special schools or classes; • where provided, specialised education and support were rendered on a racial basis, with the best human, physical and material resources reserved for the white population; j • most learners with disabilities were either excluded from the system or were mainstreamed by default; • the curriculum and educational system as a whole, generally failed to respond to the diverse needs of the learner population with disabilities and this resulted in massive numbers of academic failures; and • although attention was given to the schooling phase with regard to "special needs and support", the other levels or bands of education were seriously neglected. the government is in the process of rectifying the abovementioned injustices to learners with disabilities and proposes an inclusive education system which aims to "... promote education for all and foster the development of inclusive and supportive centres of learning that would enable all learners to participate actively in the education process so that they could develop and extend their potential and participate as equal members of society" (education white paper no. 6, 2001 p5). the south african education white paper no. 6 (2001) states that the inclusive education system will have a variety of different placements ranging from ordinary schools to special schools/resource centres with the goal of uncovering and addressing barriers to learning, and recognising and accommodating the diverse learning needs among learners. the inclusive education system will have a wider spread of educational support services that will be created in line with what learners with their specific disabilities require. schools will be divided into three categories: ordinary schools, full-service schools and special schools/resource centres. however, these three categories of placement are by no means an attempt to revert to the previous education system of separation of children with disabilities from other regular children. the difference lies in the placement strategy: learners are classified according to their need for support and not according to their physical limitations. the current movement toward inclusion of children with disabilities, including those with hearing loss, is likely to have far-reaching consequences for teachers, parents and learners (keith & ross, 1998). more specifically, the teacher, in order to successfully fulfill his/her role, will have to confront many challenges related to the audiological and educational management of children with hearing loss (english, 1995). the rationale for this statement is based on the complexities and/or characteristics related to the disability of a hearing loss (johnson, benson & seaton, 1997). children, except those exclusively immersed in signing environments, learn language primarily through the auditory pathways (english, 1995; lynas, 1994). therefore, a hearing loss often negatively impacts on the development of these children's auditory, language, speech, communication, literacy, academic, and psychosocial skills within the inclusive environment (johnson et al., 1997). children with hearing loss who are primarily educated within a signing environment will also be affected by the transition to an inclusive education system due to, amongst other reasons, communication barriers that may exist between them and their hearing peers and teachers (moores, 1996). the educational audiologist is uniquely skilled in manathe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw ging the effects of hearing loss on the child's educational development, and is a crucial member on the educational team. the educational audiologist, as specialist in the management of children with hearing loss, is able to offer a wide range of support and assistance to teachers as well as to these children in the inclusive education system (johnson et al., 1997). when teachers receive appropriate educational audiology services, they are enabled to provide quality education that strives to maximize the full potential of every child with hearing loss (english, 1995). the responsibilities of the educational audiologist continues to evolve and mature in response to changing educational trends and evolving government policy (english, 1995). the most recent guidelines for audiology services in schools as proposed by the american speech-language-hearing association are prevention of hearing loss, conservation of hearing, assessment, habilitation and amplification of the child with hearing loss, education and training of the educational team, assistance and support to the child and family, monitoring and follow-up of clients, and research in the field of educational audiology to stay abreast of new trends. (asha, 1993). it is understandable that transition to an inclusive system will not be successful without additional research and training efforts. as a possible first step in this process, it is important to determine the opinions, ideas and plans of teachers for children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system. more specifically, the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss have to be determined in order to seek solutions to provide support to teachers in their new role. supporting the teacher through educational audiology services will enhance the quality of education for all children with hearing loss (english, 1995; johnson et al., 1997; webster & wood, 1989). method aims and objectives the aim of the study was to determine the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss regarding their audiological and educational management within the inclusive education system. in order to achieve this aim, the following objectives were formulated: • to determine and describe the needs of teachers regarding their knowledge of educational audiology, and • to determine and describe the needs of teachers regarding the audiological and educational management of children with hearing loss. research design the research design was a qualitative paradigm that was descriptive and contextual in nature (leedy & ormrod, 2001; mouton & marais, 1996; schurink, 1998). a qualitative analysis of teachers' needs regarding the management of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system, made it possible to determine their current needs. by means of quantitative analysis, findings could be interpreted in terms of their generalizability to the whole population of teachers of children with hearing loss in south africa. the primary research protocol comprised of a descriptive survey by questionnaire followed by supplementary focus group interviews (stewart & shamdasani, 1990). the use of a combination of research methods had the potential of enhancing the quality of data collection and reducing the chance of, bias (berg, 1998). utilising different methods enabled the formation of a comprehensive depiction of the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within an inclusive education system. selection and description of participants the procedures for selection and the description of schools and teachers follow. selection and description of schools all 35 schools currently providing for children with hearing loss in the nine provinces of south africa were identified from a list obtained from the deaf federation of south africa (deafsa, 2001a) and were included in the questionnaire survey (see appendix a). all these schools were targeted, to ensure that the results obtained were representative of teachers of children with hearing loss in south africa (reid & gough, 2000). schools that focused exclusively on the pre-school phase were excluded. these schools were excluded because they are primarily privately owned and may not be directly affected by plans for the transition to an inclusive education system (education white paper no. 6, 2001). two gauteng schools on the list of 35 schools were purposefully (leedy & ormrod, 2001) selected for participation in the focus group interviews. purposeful selection facilitated analysis of differences between pre-determined heterogeneous groups (morgan, 1997) namely, schools that mainly promote spoken language and schools that mainly promote sign language. questionnaires (see appendix b) were sent to 32 of the total of 35 schools, thereby excluding the three schools used during the pilot study and in the focus group interviews, in order to avoid data-contamination (neuman, 1997). after completion of the survey, 27 of the 32 schools (84%) returned their questionnaires. of the total number of schools providing for children with hearing loss in south africa, 27 of the 35 schools (77%) participated in the survey. the schools in the survey represented all eight provinces that had schools for children with hearing loss, namely: eastern cape, free state, gauteng, kwazultinatal, limpopo, mpumalanga, north west, and western cape. this sample was a very good statistical representation of tlie total population of schools in south africa (huysamen, 1998).; table 1 provides a description of the schools included in the questionnaire survey. (refer to page 24) selection and description of teachers the procedures for selection are described followed by, a description of the participants. participants in the questionnaire survey y·' the participants had to be employed as teachers by any of the schools mentioned above in order to ensure that they had teaching experience with children with hearing loss and that they were familiar with the educational-setting in south africa. questionnaires were sent to 664 of the total population of 769 teachers of the 32 schools, thereby excluding the participants die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 2 5 table 1: description of schools participating in the questionnaire survey (n=32) province school nature of communication educational teachers number of school instructional audiologists educating learners approach posted at school children with hearing loss with hearing loss eastern cape 1 hearing loss sign language 0 38 300 2 hearing loss & visual impairment sign language 1 35 120 3 regular school & unit for hearing loss oral-aural 0 5 40 4 hearing loss total communication 1 vacancy 11 110 free state 5 hearing loss & visual impairment sign language 1 vacancy 16 160 6 hearing loss & visual impairment sign language 0 20 210 gauteng 7 hearing loss, visual impairment & physical impairment total communication 2 14 40 8 total communication 1 16 102 9 1 vacancy 22 170 10 hearing loss sign language 1 25 161 11 oral-aural 0 35 189 kwazulu-natal 12 hearing loss 1 vacancy 20 150 13 total communication 1 14 108 14 1 18 265 15 2 28 240 16 1 vacancy 30 275 17 sign language 1 27 241 18 0 3 12 19 bilingual/bicultural 1 25 99 limpopo 21 hearing loss & visual impairment 1 16 189 / / / 23 hearing loss, visual impairment & physical impairment sign language 1 vacancy 21 197 24 hearing loss 1 23 240 22 oral-aural 0 2 36 mpumalanga 20 hearing loss, visual impairment & cognitive impairment total communication 0 6 60 northern cape no schools providing for children with hearing loss to date north west 25 hearing loss sign language 0 7 60 26 hearing loss total communication 0 30 300 western cape 27 hearing loss total communication 1 30 200 28 hearing loss oral-aural 1 15 70 29 hearing loss total communication 2 15 200 30 hearing loss oral-aural 1 13 84 31 hearing loss total communication 1 29 150 32 hearing loss oral-aural 2 55 506 total: 664 teachers 6215 learners the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw used during the pilot study and focus group interviews. a return rate of 364 (55%) completed questionnaires was achieved. this return rate is considered good as the general return rate for mailed questionnaires is usually in the region of 20% (berg, 1998). three hundred and sixty four of the total population of 769 teachers (47%) providing for children with hearing loss in south africa participated in the questionnaire survey. the sample consisted of 75 of the 165 teachers (45%) who mainly promote spoken language, and 289 of the 604 teachers (48%) who mainly promote sign language. it was concluded that the sample was representative of the total population of teachers providing for children with hearing loss in south africa considering the following (huysamen, 1998): • participants represented all eight provinces that have schools providing for children with hearing loss in south africa; • participants represented 77% of all the schools providing for children with hearing loss; • participants from the sub-groups representing the two communication instructional approaches were represented nearly equally percentage-wise; and • a large number of completed questionnaires (n=364) were received from participants. figure 1 provides a summary of the description of participants included in the questionnaire survey. male (21%)' female (79%) 20-30 years (14%)~2 31-40 years (32%) gender age oiler (1%) sign language (1%)" sepadi (1%) tswana (3%) eng & afr sotho(7%; xhosa (11%) zulu (20%) english (27%) afrikaans (25%) other(1%) signedengiish(1%)· afrikaans (10%!aig &afr (10%) english (31%) home language medium of language instruction no response (1%)specialised deyee(2%j; no preparation (18%) every 3 months (4%)η every 6 months ( 5 % ) " > v p every 2 years-v \ \ \ ~ n o in-service (12%) / \ \ i \ (30%) annually(23%) "^-monthly (26%) 4-5% years (6%)-, 0-1'/» years 2-314 vears j f j f c s l , (13%) n m t f l j p > 5% years ' (72%) in-service training experience other combinations (4%) ί pre-school (4%)vocational (10%) no response (4%) 41-50 learners (9%) 11-20 learners (27%) phases taught teacher/learner ratio figure 1: description of participants in the questionnaire survey (n=364) participants in focus group interviews participants were randomly selected from both the junior and senior phases to ensure representativeness during the focus group interviews (leedy & ormrod, 2001). two focus groups were conducted with each selected school, totaling four separate focus group interviews. one school was representative of teachers mainly promoting spoken language and the other school represented teachers who mainly promote sign language. five participants were randomly selected from the junior phase of a school (ranging from preschool to grade 6) for the first focus group interview. for the purposes of the second focus group interview, five participants were randomly selected from the senior phase of a school (ranging from grade 7 to vocational phase). these selection procedures allowed for a more equal distribution of participants among the teaching phases. the selection of five teachers per focus group interview was regarded as a sufficient number of participants, because according to morgan (1997), a smaller number of participants are required if the participants have a high level of involvement with the topic and a smaller group allows the researcher to exercise more control over the active involvement of each participant. furthermore, five teachers were selected for each focus group interview, because findings from the pilot study revealed this to be a desirable number of participants for active focus group participation. teachers who met the selection criteria and acted as participants in the focus group interviews, are described in figure 2. / ' data collection instruments a questionnaire (see appendix b) and focus group interviews served as data collection instruments for the study. qualifications specialised training die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 27 male (5%) . "^female (95%) gender english ( " k l ^ n s afrikaans (89%) 20-30 yearn (5%)-, 31-40 years/ ;"~λ (30%) ~ f \ >. >51 years v(33%) ^41-50 yeare (32%) age univi -i degn ·· f \ j βί&ί. ~\/vhigher diploma1 honours (74%) home language qualifications specialised . —^ diploma ( no specialised — diploma (79%) specialised training 0-5% years (10%) 6-10% yeare —f \ (11%) 21-30% year / (16%) x & f y/*-11-20% year! (63%) every 6 months,—• (26%) —monthly j (48%) every 3 mont~nquu£| (26%) 1 ^ j ™ in-service training vocational (5%), pre-school(11%)>̂ y flb^j senior (31 %)^§ibj|l ik intermediate / / (53%) experience phases taught figure 2: description of participants in focus group interviews (n=19) the questionnaire a questionnaire, as a data collection instrument, was compiled for this study and considered the most advantageous for the following reasons (berg, 1998; neuman, 1997): • a wide geographical area (in this case the whole of south africa) could be included in the survey; • questionnaire surveys are more time-effective, since a large number of responses can be obtained in a limited period of time; ° questionnaire surveys are more cost-effective in comparison with face-to-face contact with participants; and • a questionnaire is completed in privacy, and participants are therefore more likely to express their true opinions and views. ' however, the main disadvantage of a mailed questionnaire is that there tends to be a poor response rate (neuman, 1997). this limitation was acknowledged, and guidelines in the literature were followed in order to facilitate a good response rate. these guidelines included, telephonic contact with principals prior, during and after the survey, sending questionnaires by courier services, and supplying postage-paid, self-addressed envelopes that were registered at the post-office, to ensure that these parcels could be tracked within the postal system (berg, 1998; neuman, 1997). the questionnaire was comprised of 30 questions distributed across 12 pages, and consisted of three sections. although a 12-page questionnaire seemed lengthy, most of the questions were closed-ended. therefore, the duration for completion (approximately 20 minutes) was considered as being within reasonable limits (berg, 1998). appendix c depicts the development and description of the questionnaire in terms of the content included and the rationale for inclusion. focus group interviews the use of focus group interviews were regarded as an important data collection instrument in the current study for the following reasons (morgan, 1997; stewart & shamdasani, 1990): • some of the quantitative results obtained from the questionnaire survey could be qualitatively interpreted; • concentrated amounts of data, on precisely the topic of interest could be extracted; • new ideas and creative concepts could be stimulated; • complex behaviours and opinions could be more clearly studied; and • participants could be made stakeholders in the research process, when they were given a chance to freely voice their feelings and suggestions. the most common disadvantage of utilising focus group interviews is that the small numbers of participants included in focus group interviews limits the generalisation of findings to the larger population (stewart & shamdasani, 1990). there: fore, this data collection method was not used in isolation in this study, but was combined with a questionnaire survey in order to make findings more generalizable. the focus group interview consisted of two topics to guide the participants during the focus group interviews (stewart & shamdasani, 1990). the first topic of discussion was: "how do you feel about the inclusive education system and children with hearing loss?". this topic remains controversial internationally amongst many teachers of children with hearing loss (english, 1995; moores, 1996). by obtaining participants' views on issues such as the challenges they foresee, and the solutions they suggest, determination of the role of the educational audiologist in attempting to address these challenges will be aided. the second topic discussed was: "how do you feel about the role of a hearing therapist (audiologist) in the inclusive education system?". by determining teachers' need for support and the challenges of current service delivery by the educational audiologist, appropriate service delivery can be planned. the duration of focus groups interviews was approximately 30 minutes each and was conducted on the school premises on separate days during in-service training time. audio recordings were made of the focus group interthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw views, in order to facilitate written transcriptions. in addition, the researcher made notes of distinct nonverbal behaviour, such as frowning, gesturing or winking, that aided the interpretation of the content of the audiocassette recordings at a later stage (stewart & shamdasani, 1990). data analysis the data analysis procedures are described. data analysis of the questionnaire survey analysis of questionnaire data' included quantitative analysis where percentages and frequencies of responses were determined, as well as qualitative analysis, where responses were described in detail. data obtained from the questionnaire survey were analyzed by means of descriptive statistics in order to describe and summarise the collection of scores obtained. descriptive statistics physically reduce large amounts of data and facilitate the drawing of conclusions about them (bless & higson-smith, 1995). raw data transferred onto spreadsheets were analyzed by means of computer software, namely sas/stat® (version 8) from the sas institute. responses to open-ended questions were abstracted into main ideas in order to categorise answers of all the participants into more manageable units (berg, 1998). data analysis of focus group interviews analysis of data obtained from the focus group interviews was qualitative in nature. the cut-and-paste technique described by stewart and shamdasani (1990) was used in order to analyze and interpret data obtained from the focus group interviews. this technique has four distinct steps that are critical in order to establish the dependability of data recording and analysis (reid & gough, 2000). the first step includes the recording of data. during the second step, the researcher identified units from the transcript that were relevant to the research aims (stewart & shamdasani, 1990). these units were underlined by means of a word processing program. in step three, the researcher reread the transcript and then identified by selecting themes that corresponded with those of the questionnaire items. the units relating to these themes were colour-coded according to the themes and were then cut and pasted into their respective classifications using a word processing program. these sorted themes provided the basis for further categorisation of content (stewart & shamdasani, 1990). finally, during step four, units that supported each theme were further categorised in order to form an interpretative representation of responses. these excerpts were numbered and were presented within a format that clearly captured the findings of each theme (stewart & shamdasani, 1990). ethical concerns research ethics define what is legitimate and moral during research procedures (neuman, 1997). according to strydom (2002), ethical issues can be divided into harm to participants, informed consent, deception of participants, violation of privacy, researcher competence, cooperation with collaborators, and release of findings. the researcher attempted to conduct herself ethically in each of these areas. participants were not harmed in a physical and/or emotional manner during the research (strydom, 2002). participants were disadvantaged if they chose not to participate in the research, and this was clearly stated to them. a covering letter accompanied by an informed consent form was provided to all participants, explaining the aims of the research, the procedures to be followed, and that participation in the study was entirely voluntary (strydom, 2002). therefore, participants were not coerced or manipulated into volunteering, and had to give informed consent in order to participate in the research project (berg, 1998). participants were also free to withdraw from the research whenever they chose to do so (strydom, 2002). the researcher ensured that participants were not deceived in any way as to the goal of the study, the purpose, the experiences that they were subjected to, or the use of the data accumulated, as these were clearly stipulated these points in a covering letter (strydom, 2002). it was essential that the researcher acted with the necessary sensitivity where privacy of participants was concerned. therefore, respondents were not requested to reveal their names. in addition, the participants were assured of the confidentiality of their responses by removing any element from the research records that may have indicated the participant's identity (berg, 1998). the researcher assured all parties involved of her competence, skill and thorough preparation to undertake the investigation at hand (strydom, 2002). the research design, data collection instruments, and procedures were reviewed by experienced research supervisors prior to the main study. prior to conducting the fieldwork, permission to carry out the research was obtained from the relevant authorities, namely: research ethics committee: faculty of humanities, university of pretoria by submitting a research proposal prior to the intended study. permission was also obtained from the various departments of education, the school principals, as well as; the participants (strydom, 2002). | questionnaires were distributed and completed during break-time so as not to interfere with regular school duties. focus group interviews were conducted after school hours during a time allocated for in-service training so as not to have caused interference with participants' regular school hours. : the participants were informed that the information obtained from the research would only be used for research purposes and would not be misused or used to cause any harm to the reputation of individual participants or to the professional groups involved (neuman, 1997). after completion of the research, a summary of findings was made available to the departments of education as well as the schools that participated in the research (strydom, 2002). results knowledge of educational audiology averages of the results were determined in order to obtain a broad overview of the findings of both sub-groups of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 2 9 table 2: knowledge of educational audiology areas of knowledge participants who mainly promote spoken language(n=75) participants who mainly promote sign language (n=289) should have knowledge requires support to obtain this knowledge should have knowledge requires support to obtain this knowledge hearing loss 85% 68% 68% 58% negative impact of a hearing loss 92% 73% 66% 64% maximising residual hearing 87% 47% 73% 42% participants on the questionnaire survey. the data is set out in table 2. some excerpts from the focus group interviews are provided in the text. hearing loss and the negative impact of a hearing loss participants of both sub-groups, namely teachers who mainly promote spoken language and teachers who mainly promote sign language, strongly recommended, that teachers have knowledge of the various aspects of hearing loss, but, a smaller number of participants, indicated that teachers required support in the acquisition of this knowledge. the teachers' need for knowledge of the various aspects of hearing loss were confirmed by two excerpts from the focus group that reveal teachers' awareness of the importance of knowledge on hearing loss. "... hearing aids are of the utmost importance, a child cannot afford to be sitting in class without an aid for even one single day..." (participant mainly promoting spoken language). "... we would have liked to know more about it (annual audiograms of pupils) ...we do know how the audiogram works, but if we could compare it with the results of the previous year... if we knew how..." (participant mainly promoting sign language) furthermore, participants who mainly promote sign language, indicated less need than the other sub-group for the acquisition of knowledge and support in the various aspects of hearing loss. j participants of both sub-groups, namely teachers who mainly promote spoken language and teachers who mainly promote sign language, recommended that teachers should have knowledge about the negative impact of a hearing loss on the various areas of development. the teachers' high regard for knowledge about the negative impact of a hearing loss is supported by focus group comments: "... our children have a big problem with abstract thinking... with maths... they experience many difficulties... they don't have insight, they are extremely bound by their concrete world..." (participant mainly promoting spoken language); "... ever so often he is embarrassed, because he didn 't do his work or know what was going on, because he didn't hear..." (participant mainly promoting sign language). an overview of results indicated that participants who mainly promote sign language indicated less need (66% vs 64%) than the other sub-group for the acquisition of knowledge and support with regard to the impact of hearing loss. further statistical analysis regarding knowledge about hearing loss and the negative impact of a hearing loss revealed that some of the variables rendered chi-square (χ2) values greater than the critical value based on ρ < .05 variables that were considered included: gender, age, teaching experience, medium of language instruction, highest educational qualification, specialised training in hearing loss, in-service training as well as the teacher/learner ratio in the classroom. chi-square values greater than the critical value indicated that these variables significantly influenced the participants' need for support in learning about hearing loss. however, none of these variables significantly influenced the participants' need for support in learning how to address the negative impact of hearing loss. participants who mainly promote spoken language, with no specialised training in hearing loss, indicated a greater need for support in learning about the classification of the types of hearing loss than participants who had received specialised training. participants who mainly promote spoken language and that have received in-service training less frequently than once per month, indicated a greater need for support in learning about audiograms and fm systems. participants who mainly promote sign language that had more than 20 learners in their classrooms indicated a greater need for support in learning about the trouble-shooting of a hearing aid. maximising of residual hearing participants of both sub-groups, namely teachers who mainly promote spoken language and teachers who mainly promote sign language, recommended (87% and 73% respectively) that teachers have knowledge about how to maximise residual hearing. however, very few participants (47% and 42% respectively) indicated that teachers required support in the acquisition of this knowledge. two excerpts from the focus group interviews confirm that teachers value knowledge on maximising residual hearing: "...the physical environment of the child should provide for his hearing impairment..." (participant mainly promoting spoken language); "...then you first have to send him back to the hostel to get his hearing aids... many of the older children have that don't-care attitude about their hearing aids..." (participant mainly promoting sign language). statistical analysis of variables and the maximising of residual hearing revealed chi-squar values greater than the critical value which indicated that the variables a significantly influenced the participants' need for support in learning how to maximise residual hearing. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw participants who mainly promote spoken language and who had diplomas, indicated a greater need for support in learning about the advocacy of fm systems in the school setting, than participants with higher qualifications. participants who mainly promote spoken language, with no specialised training in hearing loss, indicated a greater need for support in learning about the instruction of speech-reading skills to children with hearing loss than participants with specialised training. participants who mainly promote spoken language, with more than ten learners in their classrooms, indicated a greater need for support in learning about the enhancement of classroom acoustics. participants who mainly promote sign language and who had diplomas as their highest qualifications, indicated a greater need for support in the acquisition of knowledge in various areas. these areas included, how to advocate the use of fm systems in the school setting, the enhancement of correct listening skills, as well as knowledge in the instruction of speech-reading skills. participants who mainly promote sign language, with more than 20 learners in their classrooms, indicated a greater need for support in learning about the instruction of speech-reading skills. participants who mainly promote sign language who had received in-service training less frequently than once per month, indicated a greater need for support in learning about the identification of noise levels, as well as learning about the encouragement of continual hearing aid use. audiological and educational management of the child with hearing loss averages were calculated in order to condense findings of both sub-groups of participants as obtained from the questionnaire survey. these averages are presented in table 3. the findings in table 3 are discussed in terms of the most outstanding features, and items viewed similarly by participants were grouped together. some focus group excerpts are presented. speech production skills from table 3 it is clear that participants of both sub-groups strongly recommended that teachers take various intervention steps in order to develop the speech production skills of the child with hearing loss. the majority of participants of both sub-groups, namely teachers who mainly promote spoken language and teachers who mainly promote sign language recommended (80% and 60%) respectively that teachers receive professional support in order to develop the speech production skills of the child with hearing loss. participants' recommendations for the development of speech production skills are echoed in the following excerpts from the focus group interviews: "... we want them (the educational audiologists) to...motivate the children to speak, it will be a great help if there is someone to monitor each child..." (participant mainly promoting spoken language); "... they (the educational audiologists) could help...with the pronunciation and forming of words in subjects, where they have to know big words..." (participant mainly promoting sign language). results revealed that participants who mainly promote spoken language generally indicated a greater need for support (80%) in the development of speech production skills than did participants who mainly promote sign language. language, communication, literacy and academic skills results indicated that participants of both sub-groups strongly recommended that teachers take various intervention steps in order to develop skills in language, communication, literacy and academics. however, only an average to belowaverage amount of support in these areas of development was deemed to be required. excerpts from the focus groups highlighted participants' commitment to the development of language skills: "... we are specifically trained to know where language starts, in other words, we know a small little thing such as eye contact... is a form of language..." (participant mainly promoting spoken language); "... definitely in the area of sign language they (the educational audiologists)... can make more contributions in terms of planning the language lessons..." (participant mainly promoting sign language). the development of communication skills was highlighted in | the knowledge of development participants who mainly promote spoken language (n=75) participants who mainly promote j sign language (n=289) j should take various intervention steps requires support to execute these steps should take various intervention steps requires support to execute these steps , speech production skills 78% 80% 75% 60% language skills 90% 53% 74% 45% communication skills 92% 57% 77% 45% / literacy & academic skills 90% 59% ' 76% , 48% psychosocial well-being 97% 17% 92% 21% ι i table 3: audiological and educational management of the child with hearing loss die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 31 following focus group excerpts: "... he may get discouraged, because of his communication it is an obstacle between him and the other (hearing) children..." (participant mainly promoting spoken language); "... if teachers at schools for the deaf are making use of sign language interpreters why can't the audiologist also make use of them? ... it would make it much easier for the child" (participant mainly promoting sign language). the importance of developing literacy skills and academic achievement were highlighted by the following excerpts from the focus group interviews: "... with subjects you must zoom-in individually, you must explain the terminology... because their vocabulary is poor, their world experiences are poor..." (participant mainly promoting spoken language); "...if they (the educational audiologists)...can overcome that bridge between sign language and written language, they will be worth their weight in gold..." (participant mainly promoting sign language). psychosocial well-being both sub-groups indicated the importance of intervention in developing the child's psychosocial well-being. in contrast, only a very small percentage specified a need for support in doing so. emphasis on the development of psychological wellbeing can be seen in the following excerpts from the focus group interviews: "... he needs to have a solid foundation, because if he's not emotionally strong, he'll drop out..." (participant mainly promoting spoken language); "... ever so often he is embarrassed, because he didn't do his work or know what is going on, because he didn't hear... he will also feel left out from the deaf community..." (participant mainly promoting sign language). statistical analysis of the results regarding the educational management of hearing loss, revealed that some of the variables rendered chi-squared values greater than the critical value. findings reveal that both sub-groups of participants who had diplomas, indicated a greater need for support in the development of language within activities of social interaction, than'participants with higher qualifications. both sub-groups of participants who have received in-service training less frequently than once per month!, indicated a greater need for support with articulation skills when planning speech production activities. finally, both sub-groups of participants who have received in-service training less frequently than once per month, indicated a greater need for support in providing opportunities for socialising and expression in the classroom. discussion knowledge of educational audiology participants' knowledge of educational audiology, as well as their need for support in the acquisition of this knowledge, is crucial in addressing the specific needs that may arise from the sensory impairment of children with hearing loss (easterbrooks & radaszewski-byrne, 1995; flexer, 1993). hearing loss and the negative impact of a hearing loss participants of both subj-groups strongly recommended that teachers have knowledge of the various aspects of hearing loss, as well as on the negative impact of a hearing loss on the various areas of development. in contrast, a smaller number of participants felt that teachers required support in the acquisition of knowledge in these two areas. these findings may indicate that participants generally did not realise the advantages of receiving support from a professional such as an educational audiologist (english, 1995; johnson et al., 1997). furthermore, participants who mainly promote sign language indicated less need for the acquisition of knowledge and support in the various aspects of hearing loss as well as the various areas of impact relating to hearing loss than the other sub-group. the literature substantiates these findings, which can be explained by the .differences in the communication instructional approaches followed by the two sub-groups (lynas, 1994; moores, 1996). participants who mainly promote sign language were less interested in acquiring knowledge in the various aspects of hearing loss, such as the anatomy and functioning of the auditory mechanism, the aim and interpretation of an audiogram, the purpose and functioning of an fm system and hearing aid, et cetera. reasons for their disinterest can be found in the education system. participants who promote sign language often view knowledge in the aforementioned areas as approaching the hearing loss as a pathology, whereas they tend to regard hearing loss as a social identity and a sub-culture that does not necessarily have to be corrected (deafsa, 2001b; lynas, 1994; moores, 1996). teachers who mainly promote sign language generally do not regard hearing loss as a condition that needs to be habilitated or which negatively influences all areas of development (deafsa, 2001b; lynas, 1994; moores, 1996). furthermore, the acquisition of sign language is not negatively affected by the presence of a hearing loss (moores, 1996). maximising of residual hearing participants of both sub-groups recommended that teachers have knowledge about the maximising of residual hearing. however very few participants indicated that teachers required support in the acquisition of this knowledge. this finding may imply that participants generally did not realise the importance of receiving support from a professional such as an educational audiologist when maximising a child's residual hearing (english, 1995; johnson et al., 1997). audiological and educational management of the child with a hearing loss providing support to participants in the audiological and educational management of the child with hearing loss is essential in order to ensure that all facets of the child with hearing loss are developed (sanders, 1988). teachers should develop all the relevant areas in order to ensure that the child reaches his/her full potential as a scholar and a human being (sanders, 1988). information about the differences between the two sub-groups' need for support in the audiological and educational management of the child with hearing loss is crucial in order to plan for appropriate support structures in the inclusive education system. speech production skills the majority of participants of both sub-groups recommended that teachers take various intervention steps and the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw receive professional support in order to develop the speech production skills of the child with hearing loss. results revealed that participants who mainly promote spoken language generally indicated a greater need for support in this development area than did participants who mainly promote sign language. these findings are supported by the literature (jamieson, 1994; lynas, 1994; moores, 1996; paul & quigley, 1994; sanders, 1988) and this, once again, relates to the differences in the communication instructional approaches followed by the two sub-groups. it is well known that teachers who mainly promote spoken language are primarily concerned with inter alia, the child's development of speech production skills in an oral environment or in inclusive settings as this is often a prerequisite for educational success (jamieson, 1994; paul & quigley, 1994; sanders, 1988). on the other hand, teachers who mainly promote sign language tend to focus on the development of sign language skills, and the development of speech production skills is usually not a priority (lynas, 1994; moores, 1996). therefore participants who mainly promote spoken language would indicate a greater need for support in the development of speech production skills. more specifically, the results revealed that participants of both sub-groups strongly recommended professional support in order to acquire knowledge about various speech instructional approaches as well as support to subsequently apply the most suitable approach. these findings are confirmed by a recent study among south african teachers of children with hearing loss, which indicated that the majority of teachers experienced speech instruction as a difficult task, and that they felt incompetent in their ability to address deficits in speech production (isaacson, 2000). in addition, both sub-groups of participants recommended that teachers monitor changes in speech intelligibility. changes in the quality of articulation, voice, pitch, et cetera, should be monitored, in order to target the appropriate sounds that the child with hearing loss is learning to pronounce correctly (froehlinger & bryant, 1981). however, results indicated that only a small number of participants in both sub-groups recommended professional support in order to monitor changes in the child's speech intelligibility. these findings may indicate that participants were of the opinion that they had sufficient skills in this area. a study among south african teachers found, however, that teachers often neglected to monitor the changes in speech intelligibility and rarely completed phonetic inventories for each child (isaacson, 2000). the fact that participants did not indicate a need for support in this area cannot be seen to indicate that they hadsufficient skills. for these reasons, educational audiologists should promote more awareness regarding the development of speech production skills. together with speech-language therapists, they are the most suitable professionals to offer the teachers support in the areas of speech assessment and intervention (english, 1995; johnson et al„ 1997; sanders, 1988). in order to address speech deficits in children with hearing loss, the teacher will need essential information on the child's phonological repertoire, as well as audiological information such as the type and degree of hearing loss, response with amplification, speech discrimination performance, listening skills, and the child's speechreading skills (johnson et al., 1997). the educational audiologist should provide varying degrees of support in the development of speech production skills that will depend on the communication instructional approach followed by the teacher. language, communication, literacy, academic skills and psychosocial well-being participants of both sub-groups strongly recommended that teachers take various intervention steps in order to develop language skills, communication skills, literacy skills, academic achievement and psychosocial well-being in children with hearing loss. in contrast, participants of both sub-groups indicated that they required only an average to below-average amount of support in the development of these skills. participants' failure to recommend support proportional to the intervention steps, may indicate that they did not realise the benefits of receiving support from a professional such as an educational audiologist in developing the language, communication and literacy skills, and in the academic achievement and psychosocial well-being of the child with a hearing loss (english, 1995; johnson et al., 1997). it is evident that educational audiologists need to advocate the range of their services to teachers, and the benefits of receiving this support in educating children with hearing loss in the inclusive educational setting. teamwork and support should be implemented in schools to benefit the whole of the educational team. it should clearly be demonstrated that the services of educational audiologists are for all teachers and children with hearing loss, regardless of the communication instructional approach followed. conclusion results indicated various needs of teachers of children with hearing loss as well as differences between the two subgroups of participants' need for support in various areas. determining these differences is crucial in order to plan for appropriate service delivery that will benefit teachers of both sub-groups and ultimately ensure that the child with hearing loss develops his/her full potential. i this study indicated a definite lack of knowledge among teachers with regard to educational audiology principles' in spite of their in-service training. it becomes clear that teachers require further training and professional support in the development of auditory, language, speech, communication, literacy, academic, and psychosocial skills (johnson et al., 1997) in order to deliver appropriate and effective services to children with hearing loss in the inclusive education system. south africa consists of a unique combination of developed and developing contexts, and this limits the relevance of educational audiology service delivery models applied in developed countries such as the usa and european countries (fair' & louw, 1999). adaptations to the services delivered by educational audiologists should be made to overcome specific south african problems and issues. these problems and issues include overcrowded classrooms and limited staff resources, the lack of parental involvement, the absence of adequate financial resources, increasing poverty, the rising htv/aids pandemic, and challenges associated'with diversity in culture and language (penn & reagan, 1995; viljoen & molefe, 2001). the literature confirms the importance of teachers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 33 receiving the support of an educational audiologist when including the child with a hearing loss. educational audiologists are specialists in the field of hearing loss and the impact of hearing loss on a child's ability to be educated among hearing peers (english, 1995; johnson et al., 1997). furthermore, the educational audiologist assumes the role of family and community liaison agent which considers the child as a unique human being within his/her social context, and therefore links the child's significant others to the educational team, in order to ensure the applicability of the child's intervention programme, thereby increasing the success of outcomes (johnson et al., 1997). only by working together as a team can the successful inclusion of children with hearing loss be achieved in south africa. as part of this team, the educational audiologist and his/her services to children with hearing loss, should be recognised: "audiologists, as professionals who are experts in the management of hearing in an educational setting, can have an enormous impact on the future of children with all types and degrees of hearing problems. indeed, thorough and insightful audiologic management can make the difference between one child with hearing loss becoming an independent, contributing citizen and another child living life on the fringe " (flexer, 1993 p.204). acknowledgements this article is based on a doctoral thesis entitled "an educational audiology service delivery model: needs of teachers of children with hearing loss", written by the first author in partial fulfillment of the requirements for a d phil degree in communication pathology at the university of pretoria, south africa under the promotership of prof r. hugo and the co-promotorship of prof b. louw. references american speech-language-hearing association. (1993). guidelines for audiology services in the schools. asha, 35, (10), 24-32. , berg, b.l. (1998). qualitative research methods for the social sciences (3rd ed.). boston: allyn & bacon. berg, f.s., blair, j.c., & benson, p.v. (1996). classroom acoustics: the problem, impact and solution. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 27(1), 16-20. bless, c., & higson-smith, c. (1995). fundamentals of social research methods: an african perspective (2nd ed.). cape town: juta. bracket!, d. (1997). intervention for children with hearing impairment in general education settings. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 28(4), 355-361. bunch, g.o. (1987). the curriculum and the hearing-impaired student: theoretical and practical considerations. boston: college-hill. crandell, c.c., & smaldino, j.j. (2000). current practices in classroom sound field fm amplification. journal of educational audiology, 8,1-11. deafsa. (2001a). contacts: schools for the deaf. retrieved april 4,2001 from http://|www.deafsa.co.za/contact.htm. deafsa. (2001b). about: deaf culture. retrieved january 11, 2002 from http://www.deafsa.co.za/about/culturel.htm. easterbrooks, s., & radaszewski-byrne, m. (1995). the development of knowledge and skill statements for teachers of students who are deaf or hard of hearing. journal of childhood communication disorders, 17(1), 20-26. education white paper no 6. (2001). special needs education: building an inclusive education and training system. pretoria, south africa: department of education. english, k.m. (1995). educational audiology across the lifespan: serving all learners with hearing impairment. baltimore: paul η brookes. fair, l., & louw, b. (1999). early communication intervention within a community-based intervention model in south africa. the south african journal of communication disorders, 46, 13-23. flexer, c. (1993). management of hearing in an educational setting. in j.g. alpiner & p.a. mccarthy (eds.), rehabilitative audiology: children and adults (2nd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. froehlinger, vj., & bryant, r.w. (1981). mainstreaming the hearing impaired child: language and reading. in v.j. froehlinger (ed.), today's hearing-impaired child: into the mainstream of education: a practical guide for preschool and elementary teachers, parents, and administrators. (pp 159-188) washington: alexander graham bell association for the deaf. huysamen, g.k. (1998). descriptive statistics for the social and behavioural sciences (3rd ed.). pretoria: van schaik. isaacson, z. (2000). a speech programme for deaf learners to be used in the classroom by teachers. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of pretoria, gauteng, south africa. jamieson, j.r. (1994). the impact of hearing impairment. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (4th ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. johnson, c.d., benson, p.v., & seaton, j.b. (1997). educational audiology handbook. san diego: singular. keith, k., & ross, e. (1998). attitudes of a group primary school teachers towards the educational inclusion of hearing-impaired learners in regular classrooms. the south african journal of communication disorders, 45, 39-50. leedy, p.d., & ormrod, j.e. (2001). practical research: planning and design. new jersey: merril. lynas, w. (1994). communication options in the education of deaf children. san diego: singular. mcanally, p.l., rose, s., & quigley, s.p. (1987). language learning practices with deaf children. boston: collegehill. moores, d.f. (1996). educating the deaf: psychology, principles, and practices. boston: houghton miffflin. morgan, d.l. (1997). focus groups as qualitative research (2nd ed.). london: sage. mouton, j., & marais, h.c. (1996). basic concepts in the methodology of the social sciences (5th ed.). pretoria: hsrc. neuman, w.l. (1997). social research methods: qualitative and quantitative approaches (3rd ed.). boston: allyn & bacon. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) http://www.deafsa.co.za/about/culturel.htm 34 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw paul, p.v., & quigley, s.p. (1994). language and deafness (2nded.). san diego: singular. penn, c., & reagan, t. (1995). on the other hand: implications of the study of south african sign language for the education of the deaf in south africa. south african journal of education, 15(2), 92-96. reid, α., & gough, s. (2000). guidelines for reporting and evaluating qualitative research: what are the alternatives? environmental education research, 6(1), 59-90. sanders, d.m. (1988). teaching deaf children: techniques and methods. boston: college-hill. schurink, e m . (1998). designing qualitative research. in a.s. de vos (ed.), research at grass roots: a primer for the caring professions, (pp 252-263) pretoria: van schaik. stewart, d.w., & shamdasani, p.n. (1990). focus groups: theory and practice. london: sage. strydom, h. (2002). ethical aspects of research. in a.s. de vos (ed.), research at grass roots: for the social sciences and human services professions (2nd ed.). (pp 23-36) pretoria: van schaik. viljoen, c.e, & molefe, r.m.l. (2001). language related problems of limited english proficiency learners in grade 1. south african journal of education, 21(2), 121-127. webster, α., & wood, d. (1989). special needs in ordinary schools: children with hearing difficulties. london: cassell. appendix a list of schools that provide for children with hearing loss in south africa (n=35) eastern cape efata school for the blind and deaf greenwood primary school reubin birin school for the hearing impaired st thomas school for the deaf free state bartimea school for the deaf and blind thiboloha school for the deaf and blind gauteng dominican school for the deaf filadelfia secondary school katlehong school for the hearing impaired mc kharbai school for the deaf t 1 v.. sizwile school for the deaf sonitus school for the hard of hearing st vincent school for the deaf transoranje school for the deaf kwazulu-natal durban school for the hearing impaired fulton school for the deaf indaleni school for the deaf kwa thintwa school for the deaf ι kwa vulindlebe school for the deaf st martin de porres comprehensive school j vn naik school for the deaf 1 vuleka school for the deaf | limpopo bosele school for the blind and deaf | nelsonskop centre for the hearing impaired tshilidzini school for the deaf yingisani school for the deaf mpumalanga silindokuhle school for the mentally retarded, blind and deaf northern cape no schools to date north west north west secondary school kutlwanong school for the deaf western cape de la bat school dominican grimley school for deaf children / dominican school for deaf children mary kihn school for partially hearing pupils noluthando institute for the deaf nuwe hoop centre for the hearing impaired die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system appendix β 35 dear teacher, please • complete all the questions • share your knowledge and opinions in detail • place a cross in the appropriate block • more than one block may be crossed where appropriate • all responses will remain highly confidential • any identifying information will be removed from the final report section a: teacher information 1. what is your gender? male female 2. what is your age? 20-30 years 31-40 years 41-50 years 51 years and older 3. what is your home language? afrikaans english sotho zulu xhosa sign language other specify: 4. what is the highest educational qualification you have obtained? 5. have you had any special training in working with children with hearing loss? yes 5.1 if yes, please specify your training: in-service training special diploma/certificate special degree 6. / approximately how long have you been involved with children with hearing loss? 0 1 years 2 3 years 4 5 years more than 5 years section b: information regarding teaching practices i 7. which phases do you teach? . / pre-school foundation intermediate senior vocational/ gr r gr 3 gr 4 gr 6 gr 7 gr 12 technical 8. what is the total number of learners you teach? 9. what medium of language instruction do you use at your school? 0-10 11-20 ' 21-30 31-40 41-50 more than learners learners learners learners learners 50 learners afrikaans english sotho zulu xhosa sign language other specify: 10. what method of communication do you use with your learners? oral-aural sign language total communication bilingual/bicultural the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw 11. have teachers at your school received any specific in-service training in terms of managing the child with a hearing loss? (e.g. any workshops, seminars, hands-on demonstrations) yes 11.1 if yes, indicate how often training takes place: every 2 years annually every six months every three months every month 12. do you think in-service training benefits you? yes no explain your answer: section c: the teacher in the future inclusive educational system dear teacher, please • the following questions require your opinion on: what you think the skill of a teacher of children with hearing loss in the future inclusive educational system should be, and whether you think the teacher will require support from a professional person who is knowledgeable in these areas ° remember you can tick more than one answer where appropriate knowledge of the child with hearing loss 13. in which of the following areas should a teacher in an inclusive educational system have basic knowledge and support in? please tick off your choices in both columns el: teacher should have basic knowledge thereof teacher requires support from a professional to obtain this knowledge understand the process of communication interaction i know about the different communication options available, namely oral-aural, sign language, total communication and the bilingual/bicultural method i know the structure and working of the ear i 1 i be able to interpret a child's audiogram (hearing graph) know the purpose and working of an fm system ( know the purpose and working of a hearing aid know how to inspect a hearing aid and detect the problem when it is not working know the common causes of hearing loss know the types of hearing loss associated with these common causes know the factors that can further damage hearing know the impact a hearing loss will have on a child's ability to be educated / / none of the above-mentioned die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 37 14. a teacher in an inclusive educational system should know that hearing loss can impact negatively on the following areas of the child and he/she will require support in the following areas: please tick off your choices in both columns el: teacher should know a hearing loss can negatively impact on this area teacher requires support to obtain knowledge on the negative impact of the hearing loss on this area language development speech production communication skills literacy skills academic achievement psychosocial development none of the above-mentioned 15. if a child in an inclusive class has been identified with a hearing loss, the teacher should have knowledge of the following in order to enhance the child's ability to hear and he/she will require support in the following areas: please tick off your choices in both columns el: teacher should have basic knowledge thereof teacher requires support from a professional to obtain this knowledge identify noise levels inside and outside the classroomtry to reduce noise levels inside and outside the classroom suggest to the relevant authorities at school that the classroom should have more absorbent surfaces such as carpets and curtains to enhance the sound quality in the classroom suggest to the relevant authorities at school that the child could benefit from the use of an fm system in class encourage the child to wear his/her hearing aids at all times teach the child the correct listening behaviour in class teach the child speech-reading (lip-reading) skills none of the above-mentioned language of the child with hearing loss 14./ which of the following steps should a teacher in an inclusive educational system have to take in order to address a child's delayed language skills (due to hearing loss) and in which of the following areas will he/she require support? please tick off your choices in both columns kl: teacher should take these steps teacher requires support from a professional to execute these steps use the hierarchy of normal language development to plan activities for language development " take into account the child's unique level of language functioning when talking to the child take into account the child's unique level of language functioning when planning the content of teaching material modify and/or adapt teaching materials, teaching techniques, and the classroom environment to meet the language needs of the child have knowledge of different language instructional approaches such as: fitzgerald key, natural approach, etc. and apply the best suited approach for the child emphasise language across all contexts in the school practise language within activities of social interaction take into account that some children may have additional language problems such as: phonological processes, second language confusion, etc. that need to be addressed none of the above-mentioned the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw speech of the child with hearing loss 17. which of the following steps should a teacher in an inclusive educational system have to take in order to address a child's deficits in speech production (due to hearing loss) and in which of the following areas will he/she require support? please tick off your choices in both columns et teacher should take these steps teacher requires support from a professional to execute these steps use the hierarchy of normal speech development to plan activities for improvement of speech intelligibility take into account the child's unique physical ability to produce sounds with his/her mouth when planning activities for improvement of speech intelligibility obtain information on the child's ability to pronounce all the sounds monitor and document changes in the faulty sounds that the child is learning to pronounce correctly have knowledge of different speech instructional approaches such as: analytical, whole, formal, multisensory, etc. and apply the approach best suited for the child take into account that some children may have additional speech problems such as: stuttering, voice problems, etc. that need to be addressed none of the above-mentioned communication of the child with hearing loss 18. which of the following steps should a teacher in an inclusive educational system have to take in order to address a child's communication difficulties (due to hearing loss) and in which of the following areas will he/she require support? please tick off your choices in both columns et teacher should take these steps teacher requires support from a professional to execute these steps expose the child to interactional experiences so that he/she is more motivated to communicate and can develop his/her communication skills apply communication repair strategies when communication breakdowns occur in class have knowledge of the communication options available to the child, either the oral-aural, sign language, total communication or bilingual/bicultural method use one of the above-mentioned communication options in class none of the above-mentioned literacy skills of the child with hearing loss j i 19. which of the following steps should a teacher in an inclusive educational system have to take in order to address a child's poor literacy skills (due to hearing loss) and in which of the following areas will he/she require support? j please tick off your choices in both columns e*3: teacher should take these steps teacher requires support from a professional ! to execute these steps j ensure that the child has acquired the basics of language before proceeding with literacy instruction identify the origin of the reading and writing errors made by the child, such as auditory discrimination problems, language problems, etc address the origin of the reading and writing errors made by the child have knowledge of different literacy instructional approaches such as: top-down or bottom-up, etc. and apply the best suited approach for the child none of the above-mentioned y die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 39 academic achievement of the child with hearing loss 20. which of the following steps should a teacher in an inclusive educational system have to take in order to address a child's poor academic achievement (due to hearing loss) and in which of the following areas will he/she require support? please tick off your choices in both columns et teacher should take these steps teacher requires support from a professional to execute these steps tailor the child's learning experience to his/her cognitive, physical, socio-emotional, and cultural level modify the curriculum of the subject by controlling the vocabulary and syntax none of the above-mentioned psychosocial development of the child with hearing loss 21. which of the following steps should a teacher in an inclusive educational system have to take in order to address a child's troublesome psychosocial development (due to hearing loss) and in which of the following areas will he/she require support? please tick off your choices in both columns et teacher should take these steps teacher requires support from a professional to execute these steps promote the child's confidence in class encourage acceptance and respect from the child's hearing classmates monitor the child's social adjustment and integration in class and intervene when necessary give opportunity for socialising and expression in class none of the above-mentioned service delivery and the child with hearing loss 22. which of the following persons should a teacher in an inclusive educational system involve during teamwork in order to successfully plan the child's educational programme? please tick off your choices in the column g2 teacher will need these person(s) on the team / ' the child with hearing loss ' the parents the speech therapist | the hearing therapist (audiologist) the social worker j the psychologist j the occupational therapist1 others, specify: none of the above-mentioned the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw 23. which one of these persons will you choose to co-ordinate the team and to liaise with other team members in order to assist the teacher of a child with hearing loss? please tick off one choice in the column κ the child with hearing loss the parents the teacher must do it himself/herself the hearing therapist (audiologist) the speech therapist the social worker the psychologist the occupational therapist none of the above-mentioned 24. if a professional who specialises in children with hearing loss can provide support to the teacher in the inclusive education system, which of the following methods of support will benefit the teacher? please tick off your choices in the column el: teacher will benefit from the following method(s) of support a once-off training session regular workshops continuous in-service training hands-on assistance when needed none of the above-mentioned 25. if a professional who specialises in children with hearing loss can provide support to the teacher in an inclusive educational system, which one of these service delivery models would you recommend? please tick off one choice in the columnel: the school employs one full-time professional to conduct services at the school the school utilises a private professional from outside the school to conduct part-time services at the school the school employs one full-time professional who receives part-time assistance from another private professional in order to conduct services at the school none of the above-mentioned. specify your own suggestion: j open-ended questions dear teacher, provide detailed explanations of your answers 26. what main duties do you think should a hearing therapist (audiologist) have at a school? 27. do you feel that the teacher in an inclusive educational system can benefit from the support of a hearing therapist (audiologist)? yes 27.1 explain your answer: die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 28. what is the biggest challenge that may face a teacher of children with hearing loss in an inclusive educational system? 29. what possible solution(s) can you suggest for the above-mentioned challenge? 30. do you think that children with hearing loss will benefit from the future inclusive educational system? yes 31. explain your answer: thank you for your time and your valuable contribution towards this research project! the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw appendix c development and description of questionnaire content section questions topic justification section a: five close-ended questions & 1 open-ended question questions 1 to 6 biographic information of participants questions were included on participants' personal characteristics such as: gender; age; home language; qualifications; specialised training; and experience in order to describe the participants included in the study, as well as to draw correlations during data analysis. section b: seven closeended questions & 1 open-ended question questions 7 to 12 information regarding teaching practices questions requested information with regards to teaching practices and included: the educational phases taught; number of learners; medium of language instruction; method of communication instruction; and in-service training in order to describe the schools included in the study, as well as to draw comparisons during data analysis. section c: fifteen closeended questions & 5 open-ended questions question 13 knowledge of the various aspects of hearing loss and the need for support to determine whether participants realised the importance of having knowledge in these areas, in order to successfully educate children with hearing loss in the inclusive educational system. in this question provision was also made for determining participants' need for support in order to acquire this knowledge, as teachers will benefit from the support of an educational audiologist in order to acquire knowledge of the child with a hearing loss (johnson, benson & seaton, 1997). question 14 knowledge of the areas that hearing loss impacts on and the need for support to determine whether participants had knowledge of all the areas that hearing loss impacted on. participants' need for support in obtaining knowledge in order to be able to address the negative impact of the hearing loss was also probed in this question. teachers may benefit from the support of an educational audiologist in addressing the negative impact of the hearing loss on the child's ability to be educated (johnson, benson & seaton, 1997). the following items were included: language development; speech production; communication skills; literacy skills; academic achievement; and psychosocial development. these areas english (1995); johnson, benson & seaton (1997); moores (1996); and sanders (1988) could be negatively impacted by a hearing loss. question 15 knowledge of the steps to be taken in order to develop the child's residual hearing and the need for support to obtain this knowledge to determine whether participants had knowledge of all the steps required to optimally develop the child's residual hearing. participants' need for support in order to obtain knowledge on how to develop a child's residual hearing was also included, because teachers may benefit from the support of an educational audiologist in this regard. (johnson, benson & seaton, 1997). question 16 knowledge of the steps to be taken in order to develop the child's language skills and the need for support to determine whether participants had knowledge of all the steps required to develop the child's language skills. participants' need for support in order to develop the child's language skills, was also included. ι 1 question 17 knowledge of the steps to be taken in order to develop the child's speech production skills and the need for support to determine whether participants had knowledge of all the steps required to develop the child's speech production skills. participants' need for support, in order to develop the child's speech production skills, was also included. j 1 question 18 knowledge of the steps to be taken in order to develop the child's communication skills and the need for support to determine whether participants had knowledge of all the steps required to develop the child's communication skills. participants' need for support in order to develop the child's communication skills was also included. question 19 knowledge of the steps to be taken in order to develop the child's literacy skills and the need for support to determine whether participants had knowledge of all the steps required to develop the child's literacy skills. participants' need for support, in order to develop the child's literacy skills, was also included. question 20 knowledge of the steps to be taken in order to promote the child's academic achievement and the need for support to determine whether participants had knowledge of all the steps required to promote the child's academic achievement. participants' need for support in order to promote the child's academic achievement was also included in this question. this was included. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the needs of teachers of children with hearing loss within the inclusive education system 4 3 appendix c continued section questions topic justification question 21 knowledge of the steps to be taken in order to develop the child's psychosocial well-being and the need for support to determine whether participants had knowledge of all the steps required to develop the child's well-being. participants' need for support, in order to develop the child's psychosocial well-being was also included. question 22 information on the selection of relevant team members for the inclusive educational system to determine which team members participants wanted to include during teamwork in the inclusive educational system. the following items were included: the child with hearing loss; the parents; the speech therapist; the educational audiologist; the social worker; the psychologist; the occupational therapist; and an option to add a person not mentioned. working with other team members is as crucial part in the success of educating the child with a hearing loss (johnson, benson & seaton, 1997). literature suggests that of all the team members involved, the child with a hearing loss, and the parents/guardians should always be involved (english, 1995). question 23 information on the selection of a team co-ordinator for teamwork in the inclusive educational system to determine participants' opinions on which person they thought should fulfil the role of team co-ordinator during teamwork in the inclusive educational system. the following items were included: the child with hearing loss; the parents; the teacher; the educational audiologist; the speech therapist; the social worker; the psychologist; or the occupational therapist. according to literature, any of these persons, except the child, can function as a team co-ordinator (english, 1995 and benson & seaton, 1997); question 24 information on the selection of methods available for teacher support in the inclusive educational system to determine participants' opinions on what methods of support they thought could benefit teachers in the inclusive educational system. the following items were included: once-off training session; regular workshops; continuous in-service training; and hands-on assistance when needed. all of these methods of support have their benefits, but, arguably, continuous in-service training may provide the most benefit to teachers, due to the higher frequency of such training sessions (english, 1995). / / / question 2 5 information on the selection of an educational audiology service delivery model for use within the inclusive educational system to determine participants' opinions on what educational audiology service delivery model they thought could benefit teachers in the inclusive educational system. the following items were included: the school-based system; the contractual agreement system; and a combination of the two systems. these three options were identified as the main educational audiology service delivery systems found in school settings, and were therefore included (johnson, benson & seaton, 1997). question 26 i i knowledge of the functions of an educational audiologist to determine (1) whether participants had knowledge of the roles and responsibilities of the educational audiologist in the school setting; (2) to determine participants' opinions on what they thought the roles and responsibilities of the educational audiologist within the inclusive educational system should be. if teachers have knowledge of the roles and responsibilities of the educational audiologist, they will be more frequently inclined to utilise this support and, as a result, the child with hearing loss will benefit from these support services (johnson, benson & seaton, 1997). question 27 1 information on the necessity and advantages of receiving support from the educational audiologist when including a child with hearing loss to determine whether (1) participants had knowledge of the advantages of receiving support from the educational audiologist in the inclusive educational system, (2) participants' opinions on what they thought the advantages of receiving support from the educational audiologist in the inclusive educational system should be. if teachers have knowledge of these advantages they will be more frequently inclined to utilise this support with resulting benefits for the child with a hearing loss. (johnson, benson & seaton, 1997). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 catherine van dijk, rene hugo and brenda louw appendix c continued section questions topic justification question 28 information on the challenges faced by teachers when including the child with hearing loss to determine participants' opinions on what they thought the challenges might be when educating the child with hearing loss in an inclusive educational system. the transition toward an inclusive educational system will undoubtedly present challenges to teachers that need to be identified. question 29 information on the possible suggestions to address these anticipated challenges to explore participants' suggestions on how to overcome the challenges they identified in question 28, as suggestions may be incorporated in a proposal for an educational audiology service delivery model. question 30 information on the advantages or disadvantages of including the child with hearing loss to determine participants' opinions on what could be the advantages or disadvantages of the inclusive educational system for the child with a hearing loss. some potential disadvantages could be addressed by an educational audiologist. the advantages identified may highlight the possible success teachers may have when educating the child with hearing loss. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors nature of publication the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, and critically evaluative theoretical and philosophical conceptual issues dealing with aspects of human communication and its disorders, service provision, training and policy. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. manuscript style and requirements preparation of manuscript articles must he accompanied by a covering letter providing the author's address, telephone and fax numbers and e-mail address. articles must be typed on a4 pages in double spacing and in a font size of 12. three print outs of the article must be submitted. one exact copy of the article on disk must be submitted. filenames must include the first author's initials and a clearly identifiable key word and must he type-written on the last line of the last page of the reference list (for retrieval purposes only). articles must not exceed 30 pages. title page the title page of one must contain: title of the article. full names of the authors. institutional affiliation. abstract of the article in the language of the article. the title page of the remaining two copies must not contain the authors' names or institutional affiliations. abstracts and key words each article must contain an abstract of no more than 200 words. all abstracts must be in english, irrespective of the language in which the article was written. each article must provide 5-7 keyf words for indexing purposes. i body of article j all contributions are required to follow strictly, the style specified in the publication manual of the american psychological association (αρα pub. man., 2001). | headings are not numbered. the order of importance is indicated as follows: j main heading in capitals and bold print. sub-headings in capitals,!bold and italic print. sub-subheadings in upper and lower case bold and italic print. sub-sub-sub-heading in upper and lower case bold print. major headings, where applicable, must be in the order of introduction, method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements and references. all paragraphs should be indented. tables, figures and illustrations all tables, figures and illustrations must be numbered and provided with titles. the title of tables, which appear above, and of figures, which appear below, must he concise but explanatory. allow for 50-75% reduction in printing of tables, figures and illustrations. each table, figure or illustration must appear on a separate page and be print ready. preferable not printed on colour printers. references references must be cited in the text by surname of the author and the date, e.g., van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, after the first occurrence, et al. may be used from the start. the names of all authors must appear in the reference list, which must be listed in strict alphabetical order in triple spacing at the end of the article. all references must be included in the list, including secondary sources, (αρα pub. man. 2001) only acceptable abbreviations of journals may be used, (see dsi-1 abstracts, october; or the world list of scientific periodicals). the number of references should not exceed much more than 30, unless specifically warranted. examples locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear disord., 48 339-341 penrod, j.p (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. davis, g. & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca: college-hill. editing articles must be revised for grammar and style prior to submission. the manuscript style of the article must be strictly according to the guidelines provided. only articles complying with the above requirements will be accepted for review. reviewing system the peer review of refereeing system is employed as a method of quality control of this publication. peer reviewers are selected by the editor based on their expertise in the field and each article is sent to two independent reviewers to assess the quality of the manuscript's scientific and technical content. the blind peer review system is employed during which the names of the author/authors are not disclosed to the reviewers. the editor retains the final responsibility for decisions regarding revision, acceptance or rejection of the manuscript. deadline for contributions 30th january each year queries, correspondence & manuscripts address to: the editor, south african journal of communication disorders south african speech-language-hearing association p.o. box 5710 the reeds 0158 south africa copyright the copyright of all articles printed in the south african journal of communication disorders is reserved by the south african speechlanguage-hearing association (saslha). do not include more than 10 tables, figures or illustrations. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 5 stuttering: can research unravel the riddle? m. marks wahlaus, ma (log) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract in spite of decades of research on stuttering there are few unequivocal findings. several reasons for this are offered and discussed. particular attention is paid to the lack of one accepted definition of stuttering. other issues concern the unit of stuttering, inter and intra-stutterer variability, the overt and covert features of stuttering, objective and subjective measures in stuttering research and the sampling of material for study. these are some of the problems which pertain to study of the nature of stuttering. they apply also to research on therapy, which presents additional challenges to the researcher. opsomming ten spyte van dekades van navorsing oor hakkel, is daar weinig onomwonde bevindings. verskeie verklarings vir hierdie verskynsel word verskafen bespreek. spesifieke aandag wordgeskenk aan diegebrek van een aanvaarde definisie van hakkel. andergeskilpunte wat bespreek word is die eenheid van hakkel, inter en intra-hakkelvariasie, die overte en koverte kenmerke van hakkel, objektiewe en subjektiewe metings wat in hakkelnavorsinq aanqewend word en die keuse van spraakmonsters wat bestudeer word. hierdie is sommige van die probleme in die studie van die aard van hakkel. dit is ook van toepassing op navorsing oor terapie, wat addisionele mtdagings aan die navorser bied. the phrase "the riddle of stuttering" coined by bluemel in 1937 has been frequently quoted to denote the manifold difficulties concerned with stuttering. van riper questions it as being simplistic: "we do not like the term because it implies a pat verbal answer and because it fails to do justice to the complexity of the disorder, j / i stuttering is more than a riddle. it is at least a complicated multidimensional jigsaw puzzle, with many pieces still missing.'j i van riper (1982), p. 1 i to those working with this difficult disorder, it seems that it might well be described in the way winston churchill explained russia in 1937 as "a-riddle wrapped in a mystery inside an enigma". the lack of final answers is in no way due to a paucity of research efforts. curlee & perkins (1984) summing up the status of understanding of stuttering in the middle of the 1980's believe that "after so many years and so much work, it may seem somewhat discouraging that our knowledge base on stuttering will rarely support unequivocal inferences or conclusions." (preface). this theme is continued by perkins' carpenter analogy where he likens the observations about stuttering to individual pieces of lumber lacking the final design to give meaning to them (1985), and by other contemporary writers such as zimmerman, who as recently as 1984 talks in terms of the "lack of progress in coming to an understanding of stuttering and its treatment." (1984, p. 131), echoing an earlier stated view that there was a need for a unifying conceptual die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieajwykings, vol. 37, 1990 framework of stuttering (zimmerman et al. 1981). there are still no definitive answers to the questions what is stuttering? what causes it? what is the cure (a word which therapists still eschew)? these are the naive, but basic verbalizations of the issues which have constantly plagued speech therapists and the stutterers with whom they work. it is the belief of the writer that the first of these questions is the most fundamental, and the answer to it the sine qua non. the other areas, those of cause and treatment, are dependent on it. if research cannot determine or uncover what stuttering is, it is axiomatic that there can be no more than speculation about its cause or causes; if any etiological factors are presumed, it is important to know on which of the facts, or observations, about stuttering they are made, and how far the jump had to be made before the causal conjecture was made. as far as remediation is concerned, the results of therapy for a condition which cannot be absolutely defined,cannot be absolutely predictable "what stuttering is and w h a t can be done to bring about its modification are inexorably linked together" (brutten 1975, p. 201). the "facts" about stuttering are the proven data. most of these have been derived from rigorously controlled studies, which are valid, reliable and replicable. it is the view of the writer that these data are scarce. for the few studies which give positive results, there are many which purport to test the same aspects, yet produce contradictory findings. to the question of why research on stuttering has not provided definitive facts, many answers may be mooted. adams (1976) discussed some common problems in the design and © sasha 1990' r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 μ. marks wahlhaus c o n d u c t of experiments i n stuttering when he considered what he described as circular definitions of hypothetical constructs, operational definitions that lack denotative specificity, research methods based on invalid assumptions about stuttering and omitted or inappropriate measurement of speech rate in operant studies of stuttering (pp. 3-9). his thinking on these problematic issues, many of which are still unresolved, is acknowledged. it is the purpose of this paper to suggest some possible reasons why results of research on stuttering are so conflicting, and why findings of what is ostensibly the same aspect are often so disparate. the discussion centres around the nature of stuttering, and stresses the issue of definition. difficulties in defining stuttering to the intelligent but naive observer it must be puzzling to find that what is an articulation defect is an articulation defect (or was, before the head of phonology emerged from the linguistic waters); that what is cleft palate speech is cleft palate speech; but that what is stuttering, is not necessarily, or not always, stuttering. the literature abounds in non-confirmed or contradictory findings. while these are often attributed to individual differences between stutterers, or to the concept that there may be different sub-groups of stutterers, much of the conflict and confusion has as its genesis the fact that there is not unanimity among researchers as to what constitutes stuttering. to date, there has not been one definition which has received universal acceptance. the direct relationship between understanding the nature of an event and being able to define it is self-evident. if authorities do not agree about what stuttering is, it is little wonder that results of studies conflict. if there is not common cause as to what constitutes stuttering, workers may be studying different things and considering them the same. the fact that, after all the years of investigation and theorizing, there is no one accepted definition of stuttering which appears to this writer to be the most dominating reason for the disparity in research findings, and for the lack of conclusions. if one researcher takes as his operational definition (and that concept brings its own hazards) that stuttering is present if a speaker repeats, another says that stuttering is present if the speaker repeats or prolongs and yet another that stuttering is present if the observer says it is, each will interpret the findings of the identical study in a different way, and come to different conclusions about the results. it would be surprising if, over the years, some definitions did not become more accepted than others. the definitions which seem to have withstood some tests of time are those implied in johnson's description of types of nonfluencies (1961) and wingate's "standard" definition (1964). it is probable that these have been used in research because of their purely descriptive nature. a researcher can relate to terms such as wholeor part-word repetitions (johnson 1961), and "... audible or silent repetitions or prolongations in the utterance of short speech elements, namely: sounds, syllables, and words of one syllable." (wingate, 1964), (even though there can, and has been controversy over the term "voluntary" which he uses to preface this description). it is easier to mark a sound, syllable or word as being stuttered on with this directive in mind, than it would to mark a word "improperly patterned in time" (van riper, 1963). that there are problems concerning definition is not new. van riper explains some of the different concepts underlying the various definitions. by virtue of the insights he gives, his views are given verbatim: "some of the definitions are merely statements of the authors' points of view with respect to the cause or nature of the disorder ... some definitions are so broad that they fail to provide proper limitations... conversely, there are definitions which are so restrictive that they exclude many persons who would be commonly called or would call themselves stutterers... other definitions are frankly descriptive lists of behaviors, overt and covert, shown by different stutterers... finally, there are the definitions consisting of descriptions of phenomena which seek to identify the essential speech characteristics that differentiate stuttering behavior from other phenomena with which it could be confused." van riper (1982), pp. 11-12 it is the opinion of the writer that many of the problematic issues surrounding definition of stuttering can be dealt with within the frame of the molecular approach to stuttering as espoused by brutten & shoemaker (1967) and brutten (1975). it would seem that a detailed description of the observable behaviours which constitute stuttering would go far in lessening the confusion. this view endorses wingate's (1964) opinion: "any definition of stuttering which treats inadequately with speech characteristics contains a serious fault, for such features are the sine qua non of stuttering... it seems evident that, in essence, we must be content for some time to come with a 'phynotypic' definition of stuttering; that is, a definition which sticks to observable facts about stuttering and excludes hypothetical predilections. it seems to me that a definition such as this is prerequisite to pursuing further rational and reasoned inquiry into the nature of the disorder." wingate (1964), p. 484-485 several experimenters have paid attention to this issue. adams (1976) considered that it was necessary to conceive of specific behaviours, stating that "... it seems reasonable to assert that phrases like a moment or instance of stuttering are virtually useless to the researcher interested in replications..." believjing that "so long as precision and replication remain integral parts of scientific and scholarly inquiry, generaf and vague definitional terminology ought to be avoided" (pp. 4-5). | i from the foregoing discussion, it can be seen that lack of con-' census about a definition of stuttering reflects researchers' diversity of perceptions about the nature of stuttering; and that what one investigator considers to be a stuttering event may not be a stuttering event for another investigator. this lack of agreement about the most basic issue that which is to be studied seriously hampers the advancement of knowledge based on scientific study. t h e unit of stuttering ^ allied to the previous discussion of definition is the issue of what comprises the unit of stuttering.^it has been relatively easy to assume that a person is stuttering on a word, and most research is carried out with this assumption. this has come to be questioned by observant clinicians, and by research finthe south african. journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s t u t t e r i n g : can research unravel the riddle? 37 dings that the fluency of stutterers can be different to that of non-stutterers. the writer has questioned whether the unit of stuttering is, indeed, the word, or whether the stutter is waiting, beneath insecure fluency "like garp's 'undertoad' " to arrest the stutterer (marks wahlhaus, 1986). wingate (1986) asks whether words per se represent the true focus of stuttering occurrence appropriately. he feels that saying that stuttering occurs on words "... or even in relation to words ..." is essentially a surface description which reflects what he considers to be only a first level of observation. he acknowledges that we have been limited to this descriptive reference, and should be cognizant of the limitations (p. 25). the fact that researchers are not sure, not only of w h a t the stutter is, but w h e r e it is, provides another area of potential misunderstanding among researchers. interand intra-stutterer variability if there is one feature which either implicitly or explicitly dogs the footsteps of experimenters, it is that of individual differences between stutterers. hard on its heels, is the factor of intrastutterer variability. researchers attempt to cope with these problems in different ways. in group design studies there are often attempts to limit variability by, for example, excluding female stutterers, or using the same proportion of male to female stutterers which are reported in the general population. some investigators restrict the age range of their subjects, other intelligence, others family history. a factor which is often controlled for is that of severity. attempts are often made to introduce different types of controls, depending on the variables to be studied. some researchers feel confident in generalizing their findings from a large number of subjects. taking individual differences into consideration, a caveat regarding generalizing must always be recognized. an alternative to group study design is the single subject, or single subjects, design. this has received scientific status within / h e behavioural sciences] having been given particular impetus with behaviour modification studies. with these designs, the researcher is able to control for the inter-subject variability, but has to take cognisance of the differences within the same subject. this is of particular relevance to stuttering where, for example, adaptation has to be taken into consideration when stuttering change is examined, or when a subject's mood changes have to be controlled for before the effects of a treatment can be assessed (aron,-1964). working within this structure, researchers are explicitly aware of their inability to generalize from their results. there does, however, seem no doubt that detailed study of one, or of a series of idividuals, adds to the corpus of knowledge on stuttering. when intra-subject investigations are considered, it is of value to keep in mind the question of consistency. studies have been directed to finding out more about the tendency of stutterers to stutter on the same word as on a previous utterance or reading. the writer found the advanced stutterers which she studied longitudinally to have behavioural consistency over time (marks wahlhaus, 1990). nevertheless, investigators should take into consideration that there is variation in a stutterer's speech under different conditions, and that findings about the nature of the one person's speech could differ form one time to the next, depending on the1 conditions of testing. overt and covert features of stuttering in his text on the nature of stuttering, van riper (1982) uses the term "phenomenology" to introduce two chapters, the one being further detailed in terms of "overt features" and the' other, "covert reactions". many speech pathologists would disagree with wingate's (1964) relegation to a subsidiary statusof his definition of associated features, "... of a more or less general or vague nature which include such things as indications or report of excitement, tension, personal reactions, feelings of attitudes." (p. 488). exclusion of covert reactions in research on stuttering is not always considered as desirable, and studies have been oriented towards the facet only, not only within the field of speech pathology, but in allied disciplines such as psychology. defining and controlling these less tangible variables present particular difficulties when conducting research in this area, with particular difficulties inherent in measurement systems. it seems that the safeguard which should be taken is an explicit awareness of the subjectivity of these features. objective and subjective measures in stuttering research measurement in behavioural research is fraught with difficulties. with developing technologies, objective measures are becoming more viable. at times objectivity is gained at the expense of meaningfulness. glottographic findings of laryngeal function may prove to be of interest in terms of physiology and possibly etiology, but do not contribute significantly to the applied aspect of speech therapy. core findings about the nature of stuttering cannot be overestimated; however, the researcher must recognise the level of his research, and the practical implications and applications of it. the more tried and tested means of assessing and describing stuttering are those which involve some type of human judgement. many considerations have to be made when this type of assessment procedure is used in stuttering research. some of these involve the number of judges, their professional or lay standing, their experience with stuttering which can involve the question of training. there are conflicting ideas in the research reports concerning the way judge agreement should be assessed, taking into account the varied results of interand intra-judge agreement under different conditions of experimentation, as well as different statistical procedures to establish agreement (marks wahlhaus, 1979, 1990). motivated decisions have to be made as to the presentation of the material to the judges. these include such objectives as to whether this is to be done visually, auditorally, or both; whether the stutters are to be considered in isolation or in the speech context; whether the judging situation is to be rigorously experimental or more clinical, among others. attention is drawn here to these aspects, as the writer believes that if these important considerations are not taken into account, results of studies involving judges cannot be compared or collated, and that failure to control for these factors can compound the confusion of findings of studies which purportedly examine the same aspects, but in fact, do so in different ways. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykinys, vol. 37. 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 μ. marks wahlhaus sampling of material for study a factor which needs to be taken into account when studies are carried out on stuttering, is the source of the stuttering data. clinical experience has shown that many stutterers show differing severity when they read and when they speak; from a molecular point of view, therapists observe that some behaviours can be present in one condition and not in the other. yet, very many studies use stutters collected from a corpus of read material (understandably, as the reading situation is more easily controlled than conversation), and explicitly or implicitly identify these with the "stutterer's speech". even more marked than the difference between speaking and reading, is the clinically realised difference between both of these and many stutterers' speech performances when under stress, for example, when talking on the phone. van riper (1982) discusses at some length his observations and views of the way speech is disrupted by stress, within the context of his view of stuttering as a disorder of timing (pp. 437-443), but there is little experimental evidence on this issue. it would be of interest to study specifically the stuttering behaviours manifested by speakers while talking under this type of stress, particularly to see if the same behaviours are present in all situations but more frequently in the stressed situation, or if different and more bizarre responses are evidenced when the stutterer is faced with a situation which has more difficult communication demands. as part of a larger study, the writer studied this aspect and found that, in several instances, the telephone situation did indeed evoke more abnormal behaviours than were evidenced in speaking or reading (marks wahlhaus, 1990). another aspect which should receive more experimental attention is the varied way in which a person stutters on words beginning with different phonemes. as has been pointed out above, recent thinking is challenging the concept that stuttering is necessarily "on" words, or even sounds. until it is proved that word beginnings and initial phonemes are not the stuttering focus, attention must be paid to them. clinical experience has demonstrated that many stutterers manifest different behaviours depending on the articulatory and phonatory features of individual sounds. as early as 1935 johnson & brown reported on stuttering in relation to various speech sounds (1935), as did brown (1938), in one of his often-quoted studies. these studies gave rise to theoretical speculation concerning the reason for the influence of various sounds on stuttering, but there has been little definitive research on this topic. a particular lack is noted within the molecular framework, which seems particularly suited for an investigation of this type. if different behaviours are manifested with the production of words beginning with different phonemes, this cannot be ignored when stuttering behaviours are studied. ideally, stutters on words beginning with the same phoneme should be compared, rather than stutters on words beginning with any phoneme. the consideration of stuttering on different phonemes has particular relevance for therapy. additional problems for research into therapy the difficulties presented above have dealt specifically with studies of stuttering behaviour. there are additional hazards encountered by the researcher who is interested in research on etiology and therapy facets, which are not within the purview of this paper.therapy, in particular, brings its own special difficulties when research is undertaken, including a multitude of patient and therapy variables. this probably explains in part the scarcity of documented research on therapy with stutterers. conclusions what has gone before has highlighted some of the confusions which are still very real in the study of stuttering. there is a need to separate what is conjectured from what has been proved, and for an awareness of how great the quantum leap there often is between theory and fact, and fact and therapy. there is an even more basic need to ensure that the facts, the data about stuttering, are unequivocal, and that each n e w piece of research can be seen as placing a valid and reliable block on the . structure which will lead to an understanding of the distressing disorder of stuttering. references adams, m.r. some common problems in the design and conduct of experiments in stuttering. journal of speech and hearing research, 41, 3-9, 1976. aron, m.l. the effects of the combination oftrifluorperazine and amylibarbitone on adult stutterers. ph. d. thesis, university of the witwatersrand, 1964. brown, s.f. a further study of stuttering in relation to various speech sounds. quarterly journal of speech, 24, 390-397, 1938. brutten, c j. stuttering: topography, assessment, and behavior-change strategies. in j. eisenson (ed.), stuttering: a second symposium. new york: harper and row, 1975. brutten, c.j. and shoemaker d j. the modification ofstutteriny. englewood cliffs, prentice hall, 1967. curlee, r.f. and perkins, w.h. (eds.) nature and treatment of stutteriny: new directions. san diego: college-hill, 1984. johnson, w. measurements of oral reading and speaking rate and disfluency of college-age male and female stutterers and nonstutterers. journal of speech and hearing disorders, monograph supplement 7, 1-20, 1961. johnson, w. and brown, s.f. stuttering in relation to various speech sounds. quarterly journal of speech, 21, 481-496, 1935. marks wahlhaus, m.judges' agreement on auditory and visual aspects o( stuttering journal ofthe south african logopedic society, 26,318, 1979. | marks wahlhaus, m.here island. paper presented at the international conference on stuttering. university of the witwatersrand, 1986. ι marks wahlhaus, m. the consistency of stuttering beha viours oyer time. ph.d. thesis (submitted), 1990. ; j perkins, w.h. horizons and beyond: confessions of a carpenter. seminars in speech and language, 6, 233-244, 1985. j van riper, c. speech correction: principles and methods (4th edn). new york: prentice hall, 1963. i van riper, c. the nature ofstutteriny (2nd ed). englewood cliffs: prentice hall, 1982. ' , wingate, m.e. a standard definition of stuttering. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 29, 484-489, 1964. wingate, m.e. adaptation, consistency and beyond: i. limitations and contradictions. journal of fluency disorders, 11, 1-36, 1986. zimmerman, g.n. articulatory dynamics of stutterers. in r.f. curlee and w.h. perkins (eds). nature and treatment ofstutteriny: new directors. san diego: college-hill, 1984. zimmerman, g.n., smith. a. and hanley, j.m. stuttering in need of a unifying conceptual framework. journal of speech and hearinq research, 46, 25-31, 1981. ' / ' / the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 37 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 55 the nature and management of communication disorders in a rural area : the role of the community speech and hearing therapy workers marguerite schneider department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract six hospitals where community speech and hearing therapy workers (cws) are working in gazankulu were visited. firstly, data were collectedfrom records, reports and case files over an 18 month period to determine the nature and prevalence of the communication disorders seen by the cws. secondly, the cws were interviewed about their work situation, organisation of their time and intervention strategies used with communicatively disordered people in order to evaluate the efficacy of their work. methodological issues requiring consideration when undertaking this type of research are discussed. the results are discussed in terms of the implications for course modifications as well as policy decisions within the profession of speech and hearing therapy. opsomming ses hospitale in gazankulu waargemeenskapspraak-engehoorterapiewerkers (g ws) werksaam is, is besoek. die aard en voorkoms van die kommmunikasieprobleme wat deur die gwbehandel word, is bepaal, deur data uit verslae, rekords en gevalsleers oor 'n periode van 18 maande te versamel. verder is daar onderhoude met die gws ten opsigte van hul werksituasie, tydsbenutting en behandelingstrategie gevoer, met die doel om werksejfekktiwiteit te evalueer. metodologiese aspekte wa t in hierdie tipe studie oorweeg moet word, word bespreek. die resultate word in terme van die implikasies vir die modifikasie van die kursus sowel as die beleidsbesluite in die spraak en gehoorterapieberoep, bespreek. it is estimated that 8 -10% of the south african population have a communication disorder (aron, 1984; penn, 1978). this figure is borne out by estimates of communication problems in other parts of the world (van riper&emerick, 1990). if one looks at the percentage of communicatively impaired people who have access to speech and hearing therapy services it becomes apparent that in south africa (aron, 1984; drew, 1982) and indeed in the developing world generally (goerdt, 1989) there is a dearth of speech and hearing services, especially in rural areas (aron, 198|4). this is also true for most health and rehabilitation services in south africa (price, 1986; donald, 1991). faced with this problem the department of speech pathology and audiology at the university of the witwatersrand implemented the two year diploma in community speech and hearing therapy in 1984. by the end of 1991, 58 students had qualified as community speech and hearing therapy workers (cws). the diploma course is "intended to train students to operate quite differently from students in the degree course. in the former the emphasis is on preventive and promotive work, equipping students to follow the principles of community work so that they can function within a primary health care model. in the degree course students concentrate on detailed, in-depth curative work with a strong research orientation" (diploma course review, 1991, p.2). the implication is that the cws should be based in the community and would only be involved in the "provision of elementary speech and hearing therapy" (south african medical & dental council, (samdc) 1985). 1 the implementation of this course, with a clear emphasis on community work, was not accidental but was coherent with a world wide move towards developing appropriate health and rehabilitation services which would overcome shortages of person power. this movement lead to the concepts of primary health care (phc) (walt & vaughan, 1981) and community based rehabilitation (cbr) (helander, nelson, mendis & goerdt, 1989) both of which advocate the use of midand primarylevel workers to ensure a more accessible and more cost effective health and rehabilitation service (helander et al„ 1989; walt & vaughan, 1981) as well as a strong emphasis on promotive and preventive services (walt & vaughan, 1981). delaney and malan (1984) have pointed out that in order to "develop a professional identity that has relevance to our situation and which will ultimately permit a professional practice which bears a direct and meaningful relationship to the population it serves,... a comprehensive understanding of the requirements of both the patient and clinician populations is necessary" (p. 76). they have suggested that this understanding will feed into the development of appropriate training. this point is echoed a number of times in the diploma course reviews written by staff of the department of speech pathology and audiology (review, 1991) in which the following issues have been highlighted. firstly, there is not sufficient information concerning the nature and prevalence of communication problems in areas of south africa other than the white middle class communities. this prevents the achievethc south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39. 1992 sasi.ha 1shi2 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 marguerite schneider ment of the necessary, comprehensive understanding of the patient population highlighted by delaney and malan (1984). secondly, there has not been any formal evaluation undertaken to assess the efficacy of the cws in dealing with communication problems. there is an annual workshop held at the university of the witwatersrand to which qualified cws are invited (review, 1986). this forum has provided the university staff with much valuable information concerning the nature of the problems (both communication disorders and workplace issues) facing the cws but it is limited in that it does not provide detailed information about the nature of the communication disorders nor evaluate the cws in their workplace. the need for further information on communication problems and for an evaluation of the cws work provided the impetus for the present study. the implementation of the diploma course was not without controversy. the criticisms levelled at the implementers, but apparently not formally documented, included fears of the courses being a second rate qualification aimed largely at blacks, because the need for services is greatest in the black areas. furthermore, the danger of setting up such a course is that it then becomes too easy for the rest of the profession to ignore the need for changing the profession to meet the needs of south africa. these criticisms raise important issues which require careful debate but which is beyond the scope of this paper. it is an easy matter to see a need. it is a more complex matter to determine a feasible methodology which will provide the necessary information in an acceptable manner. the adoption of the phc and cbr approaches has led to a move away from using traditional methodologies for the evaluation of these approaches. miles (1991) has described this development in pakistan where lack of baseline data, trained research assistants and limited access to updated literature has encouraged the use of innovative and unorthodox methodologies which produce meaningful results for the communities concerned. miles (1991) has made the further point that there are "complex multi-causal relations discernible in rehabilitation studies" which require "flexible and holistic approaches" (p.2). in a similar vein, feuerstein (1986) has described a participatory evaluation methodology where the "approaches are ... tailored to suit the real contexts of development programmes, and the abilities and technical levels of the participants" (p.ix). the two methodological approaches described above form one end of a spectrum of "social, non-traditional" (miles, 1991) research methods which move from strongly community-based, participatory approaches through to more externally controlled epidemiological approaches. both ends of the spectrum have their advantages and disadvantages. the participatory model allows for strong community involvement in the research and therefore enhances the commitment of the community to the research and its findings (feuerstein, 1986). however, the source of data (eg. records, files) is often problematic as there are inconsistencies and gaps in the records . especially if these are not seen as a priority (miles, 1991). the uses of epidemiological research include determining the extent of health problems in the community, developing the basis for prevention programmes, and evaluating the effectiveness of preventive or therapeutic programmes (yach & botha, 1986). yach and botha (1986) have listed three types of epidemiological study, viz. descriptive, analytical and intervention. a descriptive study aims to quantify the size or extent of a health problem; an analytical study looks at why a health problem exists; and an intervention study is used to assess the effectiveness of a treatment or modification. in ideal circumstances, health planning should start with a descriptive study, move on to an analytic one and end with an intervention study (yach & botha, 1987). this process should also be applied to the field of rehabilitation. a well documented method of doing descriptive studies is the survey method (yach & botha, 1987) which provides a well structured method for data collection. miles (undated) has provided an insightful and critical review of the "uses and abuses of surveys in service development". she has stated that disability surveys are often undertaken by outside agencies as health planners believe that these will help in the development of services for disabled people. her criticisms are that there is already sufficient relevant data for planning purposes and that further data should be obtained through the existing services and notbefore services are set up. furthermore, she has noted that "surveys can be wasteful or even counter productive (and) service development should take priority" (p. 1). having set out the history and aims of the diploma course and having outlined a number of methodological considerations, i will now contextualise the present study. the purpose of the study is to address the needs discussed above : the need for further information concerning the nature and prevalence of communication disorders and the need for a formal evaluation of the work of the qualified cws. the information obtained could then be used in modifying the diploma course where necessary as well as informing rehabilitation policy development. method in view of the methodological issues discussed above, it was necessary to use a methodology which incorporated aspects of both descriptive study and participatory evaluation methods. the descriptive study method (yach & botha, 1986) was used for looking at the nature and prevalence of communication problems; and aspects of participatory research (feuerstein, 1986) were used for evaluating the efficacy of the cws, viz. the use of tape recorded interviews and discussions, observations and perusal of written information such as records, case files and departmental reports. the interviews and discussions were used as a means of involving the cws in the evaluation process so that the results would then be more meaningful and useful to them. the study does not fall into the category of 'wasteful surveys' as described by miles (undated) as it started from the existing service.this however, was not unproblematic as it provided information only on those disorders .presenting themselves to the cws and not the comprehensive information that would have been provided by a full) survey. furthermore, although the term 'prevalence' is used, it is used in a broad sense and not in a strict epidemiological sense (gerber, 1990). this is recognised as a limitation. the advantage of limiting the source of data to those disorders presenting them1 selves to the cws is that all the records could be included without having to take a sample. aims the aims of this study are twofold: 1. to determine the nature and prevalence of communication disorders seen by the cws in a rural area 2. to evaluate the efficacy of cws in a rurakarea. the choice of gazankulu as the rural area for study was made because of two factors. firstly, the wits rural facility, an extension of the university of the witwatersrand, is situated on the border of gazankulu, which makes this site accessible. secondly, there were at the time of the study, 13 cws working at 6 hospitals in gazankulu, the tsonga / shangaan homeland the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the nature and management of communication disorders in a rural area 57 situated in the north eastern transvaal, south africa. there were two cws based at each hospital except for one hospital which had 3 cws. this is also the hospital which has had a speech and hearing service since 1986 when one of the first cws to qualify started working there. at the remaining hospitals the speech and hearing services had only been in existence for 18 months prior to the collection of the data. all the cws are tsonga speakers and many of them also speak northern sotho. a pilot study was conducted to ascertain the feasibility of obtaining the necessary information from the hospital records, reports and case files. the records of one of the hospitals were examined and it was clear from this that the records kept by the cws would be a useful data collection source. following this a meeting was held with the cws in gazankulu to discuss the study and its aims and to obtain their consent to participate in the study. data collection this took place during two days spent at each of the 6 hospitals. all records and reports for all cases seen by the cws at the hospital, schools and outreach clinics were collected and photocopied for later data analysis; therapy files were perused to determine the type of therapy being given; and therapy and assessment sessions were observed. formal interviews of the cws were tape recorded for later transcription. the interview included a set number of areas for discussion as well as any other issues which arose outside of these. the areas covered dealt with the organisation of their work and time allocation for various functions (eg. travelling, therapy, ward rounds, community education), problems encountered in the management of various disorders, strengths and weakness of the training and issues of career development and professional advancement. any queries which i had from looking at the records were clarified during the time spent with the cws. a number of problems was highlighted during the data collection. the records kept by the cws lacked consistency in terms of the data given, for example, information concerning age, and diagnosis, was often omitted. hence there was a number of cases that had to be categorised as 'unspecified' in terms of age and diagnosis. there were records of disorders seen at a school or outreach clinic but no indication was given of the total number of people screened. if this was the case in only one or two instances for a hospital the average number of people screened at the other clinics and schools by the same hospital, was taken. lastly, there were some diagnostic categories (eg. speech disorders, hearing) which are very broad and require further clarification. | definitions of categories can be seen in appendix a. despite these shortcomings the data obtained were sufficiently· precise to allow clear trends to emerge. the data could have been improved if regular visits to the cws during the 18 month period had been undertaken to assess and advise on record keeping. this however was not possible due to geographical distances involved. the time scale chosen for the study was the 18 month period starting in january 1990, the time at which most of the hospitals started providing speech and hearing therapy services, and ending in july 1991. all the data was collected during august and september 1991 by me. as the records from all the hospitals showed similar trends the data from all the hospitals were pooled and analyzed in the following manner: 1. for the hospital records (inand out-patients) the number of cases of each disorder seen was calculated relative to the total number of cases seen giving a proportional figure 2. for the clinics and schools, sufficient information was available on the total number of people screened and the number of these with a disorder allowing for a prevalence rate to be calculated. the data were analyzed for three age groups: birth to 10 years, 11 to 16 years and above 16 years. this division was used to capture the differences between the high risk younger age group [high risk for hearing disorders especially middle ear infections (mcpherson & holborow, 1981 1983; who, 1992) and speech and language delays]; the lower risk adolescent group; and the adult group [at risk for the whole gamut of acquired communication disorders]. a fourth category, 'unspecified', was used for all the record entries which did not provide ages. results 1. the nature and prevalence of communication disorders a) hospitals. table 1 shows the types of disorders seen at the 6 hospitals (including both inand out-patients) and the proportion of each type of disorder relative to the total number table 1: type of problems and proportion of each relative to the total number of patients seen in β hospitals in the period january 1990 to july 1991 type of problem number of cases percentage relative to total ν = 2305 hearing 1040 45.1 hearing rechecks 167 7.3 speech 90 3.9 cerebral palsy 71 3.1 mental retardation 66 2.9 aphasia 58 2.5 speech & language 41 1.8 hearing aid fitting 37 1.6 learning problem 34 1.5 other 25 1.1 mr + cp 17 0.8 voice 15 0.7 closed head injury 14 0.6 stuttering 14 0.6 unspecified assessments 8 0.3 cleft palate 6 0.3 non-verbal 4 0.2 learning rechecks 3 0.1 filling forms 579 25.0 key: mr = mental retardation cp = cerebral palsy the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 marguerite schneider of patients seen during the period january 1990 to july 1991. from the table it is clear that hearing disorders are the most common types of disorders seen by the cws at the hospitals. these include conductive, middle ear problems as well as sensori-neural problems. speech disorders, cerebral palsy (cp), mental retardation (mr), adult acquired aphasia, speech and language disorders (predominantly in children), and "learning problems", a category the cws use to signify children not coping at school, form the bulk of the remaining disorders. voice, stuttering, closed head injury, and cleft palate are not common problems. of the total number of cases seen, 25% comprised people who came for "completion of forms". these forms are for disability grants or, more commonly, application forms for a teacher training college which requires candidates to provide information on their hearing and vision status. most of these occurred at one hospital. finally, as expected, the child (birth to 10 years) and adult groups (older than 16 years) yielded the greatest numbers of disorders. not only were patients of the "one off" category seen, but a number of patients was seen for therapy at the hospital. these were ward in-patients with aphasia or dysarthria, as well as some outpatients, mainly stutterers and speech and language impaired children. very few patients however, attended regularly for therapy. b) outreach clinics. these include visiting points, where there is no building and without a nurse in permanent attendance, and clinics where there is a clinic building with a nurse in attendance. table 2 gives details of the types of disorders seen in the clinics as well as the total number of disorders relative to the total number of people screened. the number of people screened refers to three hospitals only out of the five who do clinic visits, as they were the only ones who provided information concerning the total number of people screened as well as details of the types of disorders. in terms of ages the majority of the people screened were young children, adult table 2: types of problems and their percentage relative to the total number of problems seen at the outreach clinics of 3 hospitals women and elderly people. hearing disorders made up 90% of the disorders seen with otitis media and impacted wax being the most common of the hearing disorders. there are a number of possible reasons for this high proportion of hearing disorders. the first relates to procedural aspects. the cws screen for hearing rather than for communication disorders generally. furthermore, the screening involves an otoscopic examination together with a few questions regarding speech, language and hearing status. secondly, this high proportion reflects the reality of disadvantaged rural areas where otitis media is clearly a significant problem (who, 1992; mcpherson & holborow, 1981-1983; brobby, 1989). c) schools. only preschools and primary schools were visited, and usually the lower classes were screened. there was often a scattering of children from the higher classes who were referred by the teachers for assessment following educational talks on communication disorders. table 3 summarises the results of school screening carried out in 38 schools. this involved only 4 of the hospitals as one hospital did not provide the necessary information for inclusion in the analysis and the last hospital does not have a policy of regular school visits. of the total number of children screened 20.16% showed some type of disorder. once more, hearing disorders account for over 90% of these. however, unlike the findings from the clinics, impacted wax accounted for 76.44% and otitis media for only 10.35% of the hearing disorders. 2. evaluation of cws efficacy many issues were raised by the cws but as they are not central to this paper they will not be presented. of significance, however, the following observations were noted from the discussions, interviews and the records. a) the cws are hospital based and go out into the community from this base. they are not community based. their 'going out into the community' involves them in activities such as the giving of educational talks, screening, and some, but table 3: total number of problems seen at 38 schools (4 hospitals) and the percentage of the different types of problems relative to the total number of problems total number of people seen 12 154 total number of people seen 12 154 total number screened 6799 1 total number of problems 1 471 percentage of total = 12.10% total number screened 6799 1 total number of problems 1 471 percentage of total = 12.10% total number of problem 1371 i percentage relative to total | number = 20.jl6% type of problem number of cases percentage of total number of problems, ν = 1471 total number of problem 1371 i percentage relative to total | number = 20.jl6% type of problem number of cases percentage of total number of problems, ν = 1471 type of problem number of cases percentage otitis media 629 42.76 impacted wax 1048 76.44 ι impacted wax 557 37.87 otitis media 142 10.35 painful ears 70 4.76 foreign body 49 3.57 foreign body 56 3.81 painful ears 34 2.48 otitis externa 51 3.47 speech & language 33 2.41 other 42 2.86 other 30 2.19 for hearing test 31 2.11 otitis externa 15 1.09 perforations 24 1.36 for hearing test 8 / 0.58 speech & language 10 0.68 mental retardation 0.44 mental retardation 1 0.07 perforation 6 0.44 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) re and mangement of communication disorders in a • imal follow up of cases in their homes. the main focus of ^ ' " e d u c a t i o n a l talks is on hearing and hearing disorders as 'hev have identified this as a high priority need. 1 b) in all but one of the hospitals, two days a week are spent • the hospital and three days a week in the community. t r a n s p o r t is a big issue as the cws often have to share transr t with other health workers and therefore, have to fit their p°ogrammes into the time frames of the other workers. if there fs no available transport they do not go out. at the hospital the cws see people referred from the doctors, nurses and other rehabilitation workers within the hospital as well as patients referred after school and clinic visits for a full assessment. these assessments were not done at the visits due to shortage of time. teachers, clinic nurses and individuals in the community also referred people to the hospital. c) the cws work together with other rehabilitation workers, viz. physiotherapy (ptas) and occupational therapy assistants (otas) on specific cases such as adult aphasic people and cp children. however, they do not have properly structured rehabilitation teams except in one hospital. at the last hospital there is a well functioning rehabilitation team comprising occupational therapists, one speech and hearing therapist and two cws, otas and ptas. the work organisation of this hospital differs from the other hospitals in that they do not focus on school and clinic visits but instead, as a rehabilitation team, develop community rehabilitation projects such as support groups for mothers of cp children. it was apparent that this hospital also had a more overtly formulated policy regarding the prioritisation of needs. an example of this is their decision to target children as their main focus of work. it is important to note that this was not the only hospital with degreed therapists. d) the cws commented on a number of issues which are important to note. the first deals with the issue of whether they should do therapy. the cws reported that they need to do therapy as they are faced with many cases requiring individual or group therapy. however, as the samdc scope (1985) specifies basic management of communication disorders, at one time it was thought to be inappropriate to teach the cws how to do therapy. although this attitude is no longer held, a number of qualified cws have missed out on this training. they reported that this affects their credibility as they are unable to deal with a disorder comprehensively once it has been identified. a second issue relates to their confidence concerning the knowledge that they acquired on the course. many of the cws said that they felt most competent dealing with hearing screening and testing but lacked confidence in dealing with language disorders. this is borne out by my observations of their work in which language assessments and therapy were rudimentary with an emphasis on motor speech work (eg. tongue exercises, oral peripheral examinations) and vocabulary enrichment. these issues have implications for the course structure and teaching methods. lastly, all the cws raised the issue of the lack of career structure for their qualification which makes them feel very insecure in terms of their jobs and professional status. they also expressed frustration concerning the lack of credits for the diploma courses as this excludes them from entering the four year degree course midstream. discussion 1. the nature and prevalence of communication disorders the most common disorders seen were hearing disorders. there are two aspects to consider when explaining this occurrence. the first is that indeed this does reflect a real situation rural area 59 where hearing disorders form a large part of the communication disorders in disadvantaged rural areas. this is borne out by research on the prevalence of hearing disorders in the developing countries which have shown high occurrences of hearing disorders (who, 1992; mcpherson & holborow, 1981 -1983) although little indication isgiven in these studies of the occurrence of hearing disorders relative to speech and language disorders. many of the cases of hearing disorders seen in the present study included chronic otitis media with resulting complications such as language delay as well as sensori-neural hearing losses. brobby (1989, p. 152) has stated that "otitis media continues to be one of the most common diseases confronting the practising paediatrician and ent surgeon in the third world and it has important economic and health care implications". this is a real problem which requires decisions in terms of management, for example, by following the suggestions made by mcpherson and holborow (1981 1983) that primary and mid-level workers be taught simple but effective methods for treating chronic middle ear disorders. impacted wax was also a common disorder diagnosed. this category was used when a dark mass of wax was observed otoscopically to be obscuring the tympanic membrane. the literature on hearing impairment in developing countries (e.g., mcpherson & holborow, 1981 1983)mention the occurrence of this problem but does not give indications of its prevalence. in most studies it seems, the wax is cleared out before further testing is done, thereby implying that it is not a serious disorder. this is indeed the case as the loss is usually of the order of20 40db hl (silman & silverman, 1991) and conductive in nature so the medical implications are not serious. however, as the prevalence was high in the school age population this disorder should not be rejected as insignificant. if a child is already at risk because of poor nutrition and poor family circumstances, and in addition has a bilateral hearing loss of 40db hl, impacted wax becomes a disorder requiring serious attention. at present the cws instil drops for impacted wax assuming that it will eventually be expelled naturally. however, the efficacy of this approach has not been proven, so an intervention study to assess this is required (yach & botha, 1986). the second aspect to consider is that more of the cws time was spent doing hearing screening and assessments than screening and assessments of speech and language disorders. the first reason for this could lie in the time factor in that screening for hearing disorders using an otoscopic examination together with a few relevant questions is much more cost effective in terms of the number of people screened than speech and language screening. however, it is doubtful whether this type of hearing screening can pick up any disorder other than middle and outer ear diseases. therefore, a development of this screening should involve some form of pure tone screening to identify sensori-neural hearing losses. the use of the liverpool screening audiometer which is a "simple, hand-held device for screening audiometry in developing countries" (mcpherson & knox, 1992) could be added to the screening protocol. a second reason for more time being spent on hearing screening could relate to comments made by the cws during the discussions, interviews and observations during data collection. the cws lack confidence in their ability to assess and remediate language disorders. they do not have access to standardised assessment procedures for tsonga children and it seems that they find it difficult to put into practice general language development principles that were taught to them in relation to english. the lack of adequate knowledge of black south african languages on the part of the lecturers makes this the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 marguerite schneider a difficult problem to overcome. this highlights the need for research to develop these assessment procedures and to determine the extent of the speech and language disorders. the cws all commented that there were many children with language disorders but that they were not seeing many of them. the cws did feel confident doing articulation assessments and therapy but one queries whether this is a high priority in the disadvantaged rural areas. other disorders such as cp and mr are common disorders seen by the cws as indicated by table 1. the diagnosis of mr was made on the basis of case history factors (eg. having failed the first year of school three times), a brief assessment of speech and language development and where possible an assessment by the only psychologist working in gazankulu. at one of the hospitals the cws mentioned that there were over 700 names of mr children waiting for some form of intervention. this is clearly an area which has to be addressed although not by the cws alone. an interesting disorder to consider is that of stuttering. it is a well-recognised speech pattern as indicated by the fact that maiiy of the black languages have a word to describe it (ttiriel, 1992). however there was a very low occurrence of this disorder in the cws case load. the explanation given by the cws is that it is not seen as a disability in the tsonga culture and therefore does not get referred for treatment. 2. evaluation of the efficacy of cws from the data presented in the tables 1, 2 and 3 it is clear that there is a need for speech and hearing services in the disadvantaged rural areas. in terms of whether the cws are being effective in their work or not, the evidence points to a positive answer. there was a notable increase in the number of cases seen from the beginning of january 1990 to july 1992. this implies a certain awareness concerning the use of speech and hearing services developing in the community generally, as well as within the health and education sectors. a further pointer to the efficacy of the cws can be found in the observations of their work. at one of the hospitals the cws had met with the local remedial teacher to develop a management policy for mildly mr children. as there are no facilities for these children they decided to enrol them in local schools but with prior discussions with the teachers involved regarding the problem and its classroom management. although it is clear that the cws are providing an effective service, there are many areas which require further development such as the assessment and management of language disordered children. lastly, i will address the issues raised by the cws concerning the career structure for the diploma and their frustrations at not being able to move onto the degree course easily. firstly, it is clear that these people are not based in the community and i will go further to say that they seem well placed in the hospital. the reasons for this are that they are trained in a highly specific area of knowledge and skill and because of that should deal with specific problems relating to their knowledge: it does not seem feasible that a cw in speech and hearing therapy can deal effectively with community development issues such as sanitation, water, and nutritional needs, as well as specific communication disorders. thus i propose that the cws be viewed as mid-level workers providing a service to patients referred by primary level workers, such as the community based rehabilitation workers (crws) or facilitators (crfs), being trained at tinstwalo hospital, eastern transvaal, and alexandra health centre near johannesburg (cornielje, undated).the cws would then provide a secondary level rehabilitation service based at a hospital or school. they would still work closely with the community and the crws/crfs in terms of back and forth referrals, and education concerning communication problems. an example of this would be a child seen by the crw for a suspected hearing loss. the crw would refer the child to the cw for a full hearing assessment and hearing aid fitting after which the child would be followed up in the community by the crw. the degreed speech and hearing therapist would provide the tertiary level of service where the cws could send patients they are unable to treat because of limited knowledge. this concept of limited knowledge relates to the idea that mid-level workers should be taught more about fewer disorders so that they become effective therapists in the disorders they see most of the time and less effective for the disorders they see infrequently. this idea is based on the assumption that there is information about which disorders are most prevalent in the various areas of south africa. this study presents information on one disadvantaged rural area only. the data given in table 1 suggest a reduction of training input on cleft palate and voice problems and an increased input for language disorders if one is training cws for the gazankulu area. finally, the issue of career structure and lack of mobility from the diploma to the degree course, demands brief attention. the insecurity of the cws is an important factor to recognise. they themselves provided examples of other health care professionals who were trained and after a few years of service were told that their professional category did not have any further role. bearing this in mind, i propose that the profession of speech and hearing therapy should take a serious look at its structure and start finding creative means to ensure good career structures and mobility from one level of worker to another. an example of how this could be done is by training all potential cws and degree therapists on a 2 or 3 year degree or diploma which would have as a focus, the development of strong clinical skills for all the major disorder types. any student who wished to expand her/his knowledge and develop a strong research, academic and managerial ability would go on with one or two years of study. these thoughts could be taken even further with the application of a similar concept to the movement of crw/ crfs from a primary level of service provision to a more specific secondary level of service provision, be it in speech and hearing, occupational or physiotherapy. conclusions ! the cws are providing a useful and effective service in dealing with communication disorders in a disadvantaged rural area. through their services the need for further development of these services has become apparent, a point clearly made by miles (undated). the onus is now on the profession of speech and hearing therapy to address the issue of the development of appropriate service provision in a creative way, looking at the needs of the patient and clinician populations, and integrating this information into relevant training for all levels of service provision. acknowledgements / i thank the cws in gazankulu for their open and welcoming attitude and for sharing their ideas with me.. the financial assistance of the institute for research development of the human sciences research council towards this research is hereby acknowledged. opinions expressed in this publication and conclusions'arrived at are those of the author and do not necessarily represent the views of the institute for research development or the human sciences research council. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) nature and management of communication disorders in a rural area 61 references aron, m. l. (1984). introduction. in m. aron, & l. le roux, (eds.), proceedings of the sasha conference on community work. johannesburg, 1 5 . brobby, g. w. (1989). personal view...strategy for prevention of deafness in the third world. tropical doctor, 19, 152 154. cornielje, h. (undated). the community based rehabilitation course at the alexandrahealth centre: initial findingsfrom a recent evaluation. unpublished mimeograph, alexandra health centre, south africa. delaney, c., & malan. k., (1984). community speech and hearing therapy: some questions before answers. in m.aron, & l. le roux, (eds.), proceedings of the sasha conference on community work in speech and hearing therapy. johannesburg, 73 84. donald, d. r. (1991). training needs in educational psychology for south african social and educational conditions. s. afr.j. psychol, 21, 38 44. drew, m. (1982 october). editorial. sasha communiphon, 1-6. feuerstein, m. (1986). partners in evaluation: evaluating development and community programmes with participants. london: macmillan & talc. gerber, s. ε. (1990). prevention: the etiology of communicative disorders in children. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall. goerdt, a. (1989). manpower for community-based rehabilitation programmes. international meeting on human resources in the field of disability. tallin, 14 22 august. united nations office at vienna centre for social development and humanitarian affairs. helander, e., mendis, p., nelson, g., & goerdt, a. (1989). training in the community far people with disabilities. geneva: world health organisation. mcpherson, b.&holborow, c.(1981 -1983). study of deafness and ear disease in west africa the gambian hearing health project. london: the commonwealth society for the deaf. mcpherson, b. & knox, e. (1992). test-retest variability using the liverpool screening audiometer in a field environment. brit. j. of audiology, 26, 139 141. miles, m. (1991). effective use of action-oriented studies in pakistan. intern. j. of rehab. research, 14, 25 35. miles, s. (undated). the uses and abuses of surveys in service development planning for the disabled in the case of lesotho. unpublished mimeograph, save the children fund, uk, regional organiser. penn, c. (19 78). speech pathology and audiology in south africa -past, present and future perspectives. in l. lanham, & k. prinsloo, (eds.), language and communication studies in south africa. cape town: oxford university press. price, m. (1986). health care as an instrument of apartheid policy in south africa. health policy and planning, 1, 158 170. department of speech pathology and audiology. (1986 & 1991). review report on the undergraduate two-year diploma in speech and hearing therapy (community work). unpublished mimeographs. johannesburg: university of the witwatersrand. south african medical and dental council. (1985,9 august). samdc regulations defining the scope ofthe profession of community speech and hearing workers. government gazette. silman, s. & silverman, c. a. (1991). auditory diagnosis: principles and applications. san diego, california: academic press. turiel, r (1992). an investigation into the beliefr and attitudes of south african indigenous healers with regard to causes and management of stuttering. unpublished undergraduate research report, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand. van riper, c. & emerick, l. (1990). speech correction 8th ed. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall. walt, g. & vaughan, p. (1981). an introduction to the primary health care approach in developing countries: a review with selected annotated references. ross institute of tropical hygiene publication no. 13, london: london school of hygiene and tropical medicine. world health organisation, (1992). formulation of guidelines for management of programmes for the prevention of deafness. meeting held in new delhi, 9-10 september, 1991. geneva: who. yach, d. & botha, h. j. l„ (1986). epidemiological research methods. part ii. descriptive studies. sa medical j., 70, 766 772. yach, d. & botha, h. j. l., (1987). epidemiological research methods. part vii. epidemiological research in health planning. sa medical j., 72, 633 636. a p p e n d i x a description of the categories used i n the analysis of the d a t a table 1 categories: ι only the ambiguous categoriesj are described. hearing = any type of hearing disorder, conductive or sensorineural [ hearing recheck = recheck after diagnosis of otitis media or impacted wax to note any improvements after treatment speech = any articulation type of disorder although it is possible that some of the children diagnosed as having speech problems might well have additional language ones learning problem = when a child fails at school or is reported to be having difficulties by the teacher a rather broad category other = any disorder not included in the categories non-verbal = when a child does not appear to verbalise at all learning rechecks = when child with a learning problem returns for a recheck table 2 and 3 categories: as these disorders were identified from a hearing screening and not a full speech, language and hearing screening the categories are predominantly concerned with hearing disorders. only the ambiguous categories are described. otitis media = red tympanic membrane and/or runny ears painful ears = as reported by the patient otitis externa = itchy ears and/or sores on the pinna other = any disorder not included in the categories for hearing test = when a hearing loss is suspected the person is referred to the hospital for a hearing test perforations = as observed on otoscopic examination; usually these are quite distinct for them to be observed by the cws speech and language = broader than for table 1 as here it includes any disorder eg. stuttering the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) performance of saenglish 1st language l speakers on central auditory processing test 15 the performance of south african english first language child speakers on a "low linguistically loaded 'central auditory processing test protocol nicole g. campbell* and wayne j. wilson* * department of communication pathology, university of pretoria, pretoria, south africa *school of health and rehabilitation sciences, university of queensland, brisbane, australia abstract the lack of standardized tests for central auditory processing disorders (capd) in south africa (sa) led to the formation of a sa capd taskforce, and the interim development of a" low linguistically loaded\ "capd test protocol using test recordings from the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment disc 2.0'. this study compared the performance of 50 sa english first language child speakers (aged 8 to 12 years of age) on this protocol, with the previously published american normative data ofbellis (1996, 2003). results with respect to predicted pass criteria as calculated by mean-2sd cutoffs, suggested that the sa speakers performed of a lower level than the american speakers by an average of 5.3% per ear for the two pair dichotic digits test, 1.9 db for the masking level difference test, 8.8% per ear for the frequency pattern test humming report, 14.5% per ear for the frequency patterns test verbal report, and 39.7%> per ear for the low pass filtered speech test. consequently, the bellis (1996, 2003) data was not considered appropriate for immediate use as normative data in sa. instead, the preliminary data provided in this study was recommended as interim normative data for sa english first language child speakers until larger scale sa normative data can be obtained. introduction this study served as a direct follow-on from the saleh, campbell and wilson (2003) study published on pages 19 to 25 of this edition of the south african journal of communication disorders. whereas the saleh et al. (2003) study investigated the performance of south african (sa) english first and second language adult speakers on the " low linguistically loaded" capd test protocol selected by the sa central auditory processing disorder (capd) taskforce, the present study investigated the performance of sa eriglish first language child speakers on selected tests from the same test protocol. / methodology aims following on from the sa capd taskforce's proposed" low linguistically loaded" interim capd test protocol, and the resulting need for sa specific normative data, this study used a comparative research design (leedy δΐ ormrod, 2001) to: obtain preliminary normative data from sa english first language child speakers, on four tests of capd suitable for use in the proposed test protocol. determine if the performances of the sa english speakers differed from the previously reported american normative data ofbellis (1996, 2003). subjects fifty child subjects (10 each from the 8, 9, 10, 11 and 12 year old age groups) were conveniently sampled from four mainstream primary schools in the gauteng region. the children were recruited by approaching the principals of the schools. permission was obtained from the principals to send letters home with the children that outlined the aim of the study, the test procedures as well as a request for volunteers to participate in the study. the sample size for each age interval was based on the minimum sample size required for utilizing the means procedure of the sas program (sas institute inc., 1999) and the time limitations of the study. the subjects all spoke sa english as their first language (based on the first language leant at school, and the first language used in general day-to-day living) and were all of the same ethnic background, namely white english first language speakers. they had no developmental or learning disabilities, no known medical history of adverse neurological or medical conditions, and normal pure tone thresholds and acoustic immittance results (hall & mueller, 1997; martin & clark, 2000). all criteria, except the pure tone and acoustic immittance requirements, were confirmed by subject and parental report only. materials and apparatus a subject information sheet and letter of consent were used to explain the purpose and nature of the study. a biographical questionnaire, a welchallyn 3.5 v hal otoset otoscope, an audiometer (interacoustic ac30 audiometer with telephonic tdh-50 earphones), an acoustic immittance meter (gsi 28a middle ear analyzer), a compact disk player (single disc philips portable ax1000), and the cid w-l list of spondees (presented live voice), were used to ensure the subjects had no history or peripheral hearing deficits that could adversely affect the capd testing. the same audiometer and compact disk player, and the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment disc 2.0' (wilson & strouse, 1998), were used to obtain the capd test data. of all the tests available on the 'tonal and the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) nicole g. campbell and wayne j. wilson 16 disc 2.0' (wilson & strouse, 1998), only the following four tests were used: the two pair dichotic digits test (a low linguistically loaded dichotic speech test), the low-pass filtered speech test (a monaural low redundancy test), the frequency patterns test (a temporal patterning test), and the speech masking level difference test (a binaural interaction test). the two pair dichotic test was selected, as this task was more challenging than the use of single digits, yet simple enough for young children (bellis, 1996, 2003). the remaining three tests were selected as the child age norms for these tests were better defined than for other tests of capd (bellis, 1996, 2003). all tests were suitable for inclusion in the" low linguistically loaded"capd test protocol as recommended by the sa capd taskforce (south africa central auditory processing taskforce, 2000, 2001). it must be noted that the low-pass filtered speech test recording used in this study was from the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment, disc 2.0' (wilson & strouse, 1998). this recording used monosyllabic words from list 3 of the northwestern university auditory test no. 6 (n. u. no. 6), spoken by a female, and low-pass filtered with a 1500hz cutoff at 115 db/octave (wilson, zizz, shanks & causey, 1990). the low-pass filtered speech test recording used by bellis (1996, 2003) was from auditec, st louis. this recording used monosyllabic words from list 3 of the northwestern university auditory test no. 6 (n. u. no. 6), spoken by a male, and low-pass filtered with a 1000hz cutoff (bellis, 1996,2003). all testing was conducted in a sound-treated test booth and all audiometric equipment and test environments complied with the south african bureau of standards specifications. procedures on arrival for testing, the parents of each subject completed the informed consent and pre-test questionnaire forms, and each subject underwent otoscopic, pure tone, speech reception and acoustic immittance testing. subjects who met the selection criteria were then tested on the selected capd tests from the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment disc 2.0' (wilson & strouse, 1998). both the order of test presentation, and the order of ear testing (where appropriate), were randomized. the test procedures used were as per those described by saleh et al. (2003) on pages 21 to 22 of this e d i t i o n of the south african j o u r n a l of c o m m u n i c a t i o n d i s o r d e r s , with the following modifications: 1) each subject was familiarised with the test stimuli prior to the tests being applied. for the twopair dichotic digits, each subject was asked to repeat sequences of four digits spoken live voice by the examiner. for the low-pass filtered speech and speech masking level difference tests, the test-words were read to each subject by the examiner and their meaning discussed. for the frequency pattern test, each subject was asked to verbally label and hum patterns as hummed by the examiner. each subject was also informed that the accent of the recorded s p e e c h m a t e r i a l was a m e r i c a n , and that the pronunciation of some of the words differed slightly from t h e sa p r o n u n c i a t i o n . w h i l s t s i m i l a r familiarisation techniques were used by bellis (1996, 2003), their use in the present study was primarily to counter the possible linguistic bias resulting from assessing sa english first language speaking children using capd test materials recorded in american english (wilson & strouse, 1998). 2) stimulus presentation levels were set to 50 dbsl relative to the average pure tone threshold at 0.5, 1 and 2 khz. these levels were different from that of saleh et al. (2003) and bellis' (1996, 2003) use of 50 dbsl relative to the 1000 hz threshold, 50 dbsl relative to spondee threshold, or 50 dbhl, depending on the test involved. table 1 summarises the differences in presentation levels between this study and those used by bellis (1996, 2003). the reason for the different levels was that the normative data being generated by this study was used in a subsequent doctoral study by campbell (2003) where spondee thresholds were not measured. 3) the frequency pattern test was presented twice. for the first presentation, each subject had to say the patterns back. for the second presentation, each subject had to hum the patterns back. the same frequency patterns and protocol were used for each response format. data collection and analysis all subject responses were recorded manually and scored off-line. group performances on each capd test were described using means and standard deviations. comparisons with the normative data of bellis (1996, 2003) were completed using mean-2sd cut-off values only. these mean 2sd values were used as they were the only values provided by bellis (1996, 2003) (her report did not report sample sizes, subject selection criteria, or raw data). | results and discussion ' table 2 shows the results obtained for the sa english first language child speakers, and the related bellis (1996, 2003) american normative data, on the capd tests assessed. compared to the american normative data (using the mean-2sd values only), the table 1. differences in the test procedures employed by this study and by bellis (1996, 2003). test audiometer settings used in current study audiometer settings used by bellis (1996, 2003) the two pair dichotic digits test 50 dbsl relative to the spondee threshold of the better ear the frequency patterns test 50 dbsl relative to the average pure tone threshold at 500, 1000 and 2000 hz of the better ear 50 dbsl relative to the 1000 hz threshold of the test ear the low-pass filtered speech test 50 dbhl die suid-afrikaans e tydskrif vir kommunikasieeafwykings, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) performance of sa english 1st language l speakers on central auditory processing test 17 table 2: sa english first language speaking child data (mean ± sd and mean 2sd), and bellis1 (1996, 2003) american normative data (mean 2sd only, in bold) age in years test dichotic digits test frequency pattern test verbal frequency pattern test • humming low-pass filtered speech speech mld test (db) ear r(%) l(%) r(%) l (%) r(%) l(%) r(%) l (%) 8 (n=10) mean ± sd mean-2sd bellis 87.0 ±7.5 71.9 75 77.3 ±8.8 59.7 65 49.4 ± 14.5 20.4 42 50.2 ± 12.9 24.5 42 56.8 ±8.8 39.2 42 56.4 ±9.7 37.0 42 43.5 + 13.1 17.2 70 37.5 ±15.5 6.5 70 5.2 + 1.1 2.9 5.5 9 (n=10) mean ± sd mean-2sd bellis 88.0 ±7.4 73.1 80 82.0 ±7.9 66.2 75 64.0 ±9.6 44.9 63 64.0 ±7.7 49.1 63 67.2 ± 6.2 54.8 63 68.2 ±6.3 55.6 63 49.5 ±9.3 31.0 68 50.5 ± 12.6 25.4 68 5.8 + 0.8 4.1 5.5 10 (n=10) mean ± sd mean-2sd bellis 93.3 ±3.6 86.2 85 90.0 + 8.2 73.7 78 73.6 ±8.5 56.7 78 72.6 ±6.0 60.7 78 77.0 ±4.3 69.3 78 75.8 + 5.2 65.4 78 52.5 + 11.6 29.3 72 54.0 ±8.8 36.5 72 5.4 ±0.9 3.7 5.5 11 (n=10) mean ± sd mean-2sd bellis 94.3 ±6.1 82.0 90 92.0 ±5.1 82,0 88 80.0 ± 4.6 81.8 78 81.2 + 5.7 70.8 78 82.4 + 5.7 69.9 78 82.8 ±4.6 71.0 78 57.0 ±9.5 38.0 75 55.0 + 8.2 38.0 75 5.8+1.7 2.4 5.5 12 (n=10) mean + sd mean-2sd bellis 93.5 ±4.6 84.3 90 92.8 ±5.1 82.6 90 82.4+10.7 61.0 80 79.6+11.4 56.8 80 84.8 ±9.2 66.4 80 82.4 ±8.3 65.9 80 69.0 ±7.8 53.5 78 67.9 ±7.1 53.8 78 6.2 ± 1.2 3.7 5.5 r right l left than bellis' (1996, 2003) american normative data for all tests except the right ear score of the 10 year olds on the two pair dichotic digits test. ranking the mean-2sd scores of the sa english first language child speakers showed that they performed most like their american english speaking counterparts on the two pair dichotic digits test (averaging 5.3% lower/ear), followed by the speech masking level difference test (averaging 1.9 db lower), the frequency pattern test humming report (averaging 8.8% lower/ear), the frequency patterns test verbal report (averaging 14.5% lower/ear), and lastly the low-pass filtered speech test (averaging 39.7% lower/ear). the most likely factor to have influenced the two pair dichotic digits, the low pass filtered speech, and the speech masking level difference test scores, was the american english accent of the test recordings. whilst thesejtests were chosen because of ,tlieir relatively low linguistic load, there was still sufficient load to disadvantage the sa english first language child speakers. an a c c e n t m i s m a t c h was not e n o u g h , however, to explain the much poorer mean-2sd scores obtained by the sa english first language child speakers on the low-pa'ss filtered speech test. this effect was more likely to^have resulted from the different cd recordings used by this study and those ofbellis (1996, 2003). it is possible that the words on this study's 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment, disc 2.0' (wilson & strouse, 1998) recording may harder to recognise (bellis, 1996, 2003). the lower scores were of particular concern when considering the very low (<20%) scores obtained by the 8 year old group, and the generally low scores (<40%) obtained by the 9 to 11 year old groups. overall, these results suggest that the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment, disc 2.0' (wilson & strouse, 1998) recording of the low-pass filtered speech test should be approached with caution when assessing sa english first language child speakers. similarly, an accent mismatch also cannot explain the lower mean-2sd scores obtained by the sa english first language subjects on the frequency patterns test on both the verbal and humming report. further research is needed to identify the reasons for these differences, although the better scores obtained on the humming report suggest a possible influence of the maturity level of the corpus callosum (bellis, 2003). similar to the performance on the low-pass filtered speech test, the very poor scores obtained by the 8 year old group on the frequency patterns test for verbal report, suggest that the frequency patterns test on the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment disc 2.0's' (wilson & strouse, 1998) should also be used with caution in this younger age group. conclusions preliminary normative data was obtained for sa english first language child speakers on four tests of capd selected from the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment disc 2.0' (wilson & strouse, 1998). each test was chosen because of its suitability for use in the" low linguistically loaded"test protocol proposed by the sa capd taskforce. on average, the sa english first language child speakers performed worse than the bellis (1996, 2003) american data (as calculated by comparing mean2sd cutoffs). as a result, the bellis (1996, 2003) a m e r i c a n n o r m a t i v e data was not c o n s i d e r e d appropriate for immediate use in sa. instead, the sa data provided in this study was recommended for use as preliminary normative data for sa english first language child speakers, until larger scale sa normative data can be obtained. in particular, the subjects lower scores on the low-pass filtered speech test contained on the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment disc 2.0' (wilson, & strouse, 1998), suggest that this test should be approached with caution when assessing south the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) nicole g. campbell and wayne j. wilson 18 limitations of this study are noted and the results cannot be generalized beyond the subject, stimulus and recording parameters used. note: the 'tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment disc 2.0' (wilson, & strouse, 1998) is available from professor richard wilson phd, audiology (126), va medical centre, mountain home, tennessee 37684, ph +1 423 926 1171 ext 7553, fax +1 423 232 6903, email r i c h a r d . w i l s o n 2 @ m e d . v a . g o v o r wilson.richard@mtn-home.va.gov. professor wilson does not charge a formal price for the cd, as it was produced by usa veterans affairs. he has requested that you send a "donation (postal order)" of us$50100 to his research fund the east tennessee state university (etsu) foundation. references bellis, t. j. (1996). assessment and management of central auditory processing disorders in the educational setting: from science to practice. san diego: singular publishing group. bellis, t. j. (2003). assessment and management of central auditory processing disorders in the educational setting: from science to practice (2nded.). san diego: singular publishing group. campbell, n. g. (2003). the central auditory processing and continuous performance of children with attention deficit hyperactivity disorder (adhd) in the medicated and nonmedicated state. u n p u b l i s h e d d o c t o r a l dissertation. university of pretoria, pretoria. hall, j. w., & mueller, h. w. (1997). audiologists' desk reference. san diego: singular publishing group. leedy, p. d., & ormrod, j. e. (2001). practical research: planning and design (7th ed.). upper saddle river, new jersey: merrill prentice hall. martin f. n., & clark, j. g. (2000). introduction to audiology (7th ed.). boston: allyn and bacon. sas institute inc. (1999). sas/stat user's guide version 8. cary, nc: sasinstitute inc. saleh, s., campbell, n. g., & wilson, w. j. (2003). the performance of south african english first and second language adult speakers on a" low linguistically loaded" central auditory processing t e s t p r o t o c o l . south african journal of communication disorders, 50, 19-25. s o u t h africa c e n t r a l a u d i t o r y p r o c e s s i n g disorder taskforce. (2000). sa capd taskforce business plan. unpublished document. pretoria, south africa. s o u t h africa c e n t r a l a u d i t o r y p r o c e s s i n g disorder taskforce. (2001). sa capd taskforce: an update. unpublished document. pretoria, south africa. wilson, r.h., & strouse, a. (1998). tonal and speech materials for auditory perceptual assessment disc 2.0. tennessee: department of veterans affairs. wilson, r. h., zizz, c. α., & sperry, j. l. (1994). masking level difference for spondaic words in 2000-msec bursts of broadband noise. journal of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieeafwykings, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mailto:richard.wilson2@med.va.gov mailto:wilson.richard@mtn-home.va.gov 49 tentative age-related norms for high frequency electric bone conduction audiometry g r voogt department of otorhinolaryngology medical university of southern africa η s schoeman department of mathematics and statistics medical university of southern africa abstract from 172 male and female patients aged 7 71 years old and admitted in one month to a tuberculosis hospital, 150 had their highest audible electric bone conduction (ebc) frequency evaluated, prior to the commencement of any medical treatment. various problems in establishing high frequency norms are discussed and from the results of this study tentative agerelated highest frequency ebc norms are suggested. possible clinical applications of evaluating the highest audible c frequency is suggested. opsomming van 172 manlike en vroulike pasiente met ouderdomme 7-71 jaar oud wat in een maand opgeneem is in 'n tuberkulosehospitaal, is 150 se hoogste hoorbare elektriese beengeleidingsfrekwensie bepaal, voor die aanvang van enige mediese behandeling. verskeie probleme in die daarstelling van hoefrekwensie-norme word bespreek en uit die resultate van hierdie studie word tentatiewe ouderdomsverwante hoogste hoorbare elektriese beengeleidingsfrekwensienorme voorgestel. moontlike kliniese toepassings van die bepaling van die hoogste hoorbare elektriese beengeleidingsfrekwensie word voorgestel. \ key words: highest audible frequency, electric bone conduction, age, sex, norms. introduction as early as 1876 galton showed interest in establishing the upper frequency hearing limits for humans, by using such crude instrumentation as whistles in his examinations (galton, 1876). ! the interest in compiling high frequency normative data dates way back to 1929, when dr. harvey fletcher's original reports on high frequency normative studies were published. at that time he reported that the data was very uncertain and varied greatly amongst individuals (fletcher, 1929). since those early years immense progress has been made in design and development of audiometers, testing procedures and calibration of the equipment. however, different researchers still reported widely differing data for high frequency hearing thresholds and thus also normative data (sivian & white, 1933; dadson & king, 1952; rudmose, 1961; zislis & fletcher, 1966; harris & myers, 1971; and northern, downs, rudmose, glorig & fletcher, 1972). thus reports on high frequency audiometry originated from research investigations but no clinical applications were suggested. 1 recently many investigators have shown renewed interest in high frequency audiometry as more and more possible clinical applications have become evident. jacobson, downs and fletcher (1969) applied high frequency audiometry for the early detection of ototoxicity; corliss, doster, simonton and downs (1970) for detecting noise-induced hearing damage; fletcher, cairns, collins and endicott (1967) for detecting hearing loss due to meningitis; rosen and olin (1968) for detecting hearing loss related to coronary heart disease. in all cases only air conduction stimulation was used. as no normative threshold data existed, much use was made of baseline threshold data to which any changes in hearing, subsequent to diseases or treatment, was compared. methods of testing, instrumentation and calibration of instrumentation also differed widely (northern et al., 1972). until 1988 high frequency audiometry was mostly restricted to studies involving sensori-neural disease processes, thus the interpretation of high frequency threshold changes have remained largely conjectural. bone conduction audiometry has been restricted to the frequency range of 250 hz to 6 khz, because bone vibrator quality is poor die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 g r voogt and η s schoeman at higher frequencies. this latter problem was largely overcome in 1984 by the development of the "electric boneconduction" audiometer which works by electro-stimulation via mylar coated electrodes on the skin over the mastoid (tbnndorf & kurman, 1984), which has excellent testretest reliability (okstad, laukli & mair, 1988) and can test up to 20 khz. despite all these developments, various factors still prevent the establishment of proven, reliable high frequency normative threshold data. consider that to calibrate a standard air conduction audiometer a 6 cm3 iec 303 coupler has to be used and the audiometer output calibrated to iso 389 (1985) standards, l b calibrate an air conduction audiometer which can test up to 20 khz, eg., the demlar 20k, a special coupler supplied by the manufacturer of this audiometer should be used. when an "electric bone-conduction" audiometer, eg., the audimax 500 has to be calibrated, the electric output must be connected to a simulated load supplied by the manufacturer of the audimax. thus there appears to be no hard and fast standard of calibration for these very different types of audiometers. in evaluation of clinical testing, further problems re use of high frequency audiometry surfaced. when comparing hearing thresholds on the same subjects using a demlar 20k and an audimax 500, okstad et al. (1988) found that this does not result in equivalent air conduction and bone conduction thresholds at all frequencies. schechter, fausti, rappaport and frey (1986) found that factors such as age, gender, different equipment, different testing procedures, headphone placement, ear canal acoustics, etc., all interact to give a great degree of intersubject variability in the high frequency hearing range. tonndorf and kurman (1984) and okstad et al. (1988) found that the normative electric bone conduction (ebc) threshold curves are relatively flat up to a variable cut-off frequency, after which there is a very sharp fall of thresholds over the subsequent 2 or 3 khz. this sharp fall indicates the point of highest audible frequency at hearing threshold level. however, it still does not indicate the real highest frequency at which the subject may be able to hear at higher stimulus intensities. furthermore, in audiometric testing the stimulus intensity scale is logarithmic, while in ebc testing it is linear. consider further that the conventional frequency scale is also logarithmic, while in ebc testing it is linear. therefore it would appear that by keeping the stimulus intensity constant and at the maximum of the audiometer, while measuring the highest audible frequency, it would lead to a more finely determined and therefore more accurate test result. recently a considerable amount of research was done on high frequency hearing and thresholds. frank (1990) examined high frequency air conduction thresholds in adults 18-28 years old; frank and dreisbach (1991) tested for repeatability of high frequency air conduction thresholds in adults 19-27 years old; schechter et al. (1986) and stelmachowicz, beauchaine, kalberer and jesteadt(1989) worked on age-related high frequency air conduction thresholds; okstad et al. (1988) compared high frequency air conduction and bone conduction thresholds in adults 20-24 years old. the differences in testing equipment, methodology and subjects result in considerable difficulties when attempting direct comparison of their results. there was found to be reasonable agreement on high frequency air conduction thresholds and norms, but not much on high frequency bone conduction. in an effort to try to establish some kind of high frequency ebc hearing norms, utilizing the suggested method of measuring the highest audible ebc frequency, a group of subjects of differing age and sex were tested at the maximum stimulus intensity of the ebc audiometer. the results may possibly then also shed more light on the pattern of loss of high frequency bone conduction hearing with increasing age and the possible effects that gender may have on this pattern. methodology all 172 patients admitted over a period of one month to a tuberculosis hospital, whether for treatment or followup examinations, (and statistically not a representative sample of the population) had their highest audible frequency evaluated at the maximum stimulus intensity of the audiometer (120 electro-stimulation units which compares to about 60 db spl). the audiometer used was an audimax 500, which essentially measures ebc hearing up to 20 khz. the highest audible ebc frequency was determined by increasing and decreasing the stimulus frequency in 100 hz steps. the point of highest audible ebc frequency was defined as that level at which the subject indicated he could just barely detect the sound, this level being crossed in three runs consecutively. as no effective masking is available, the test results indicated the hearing binaurally. the hospital is built on a vast expanse of open field, resulting in very quiet surroundings. the hearing tests were performed in a large and unused dental examination room which is situated a considerable distance away from the main hospital buildings, resulting in an extremely quiet test environment. the subjects included different indigenous races, male and female, with age ranging from 7 to 71 yrs old. from these subjects, 22 had to be excluded because they displayed poor hearing/no hearing in the 8-20 khz range, had middle ear problems (determined by standard audiometry and impedance tests), were previously treated with ototoxic drugs or had previous exposure to occupational industrial noise (determined by examining case :histories and questioning subjects). the subjects were grouped into ten year age intervals, mainly to be able to compare the results with those from other researchers at a later stage. each subject was tested twice, on consecutive days, in order to make sureithat the test results had test-retest repeatability, differences not exceeding 200 hz. the average of the two measurements on each subject was then considered as the highest audible ebc frequency. these test results were then cross referenced with age and gender. results figure 1 shows a plotting of the mean values of the highest audible ebc frequencies, calculated from the raw data, for males, females, and males and females together, set out in ten year age groups. although males showed slightly higher mean values than females for all age groups, (see fig. 1) this difference between the two sexes was not statistically significant. consequently, the data for males and females were lumped together. statistical data from the test results for males and females together are displayed in table 1. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) i t e n t a t i v e age-related norms for high frequency electric bone conduction audiometry prom ibble 1 a definite but gradual decrease is observed 51 • mean highest audible ebc frequency with increasing 1 1 1 the youngest age group constituted one subject only, w?o c o u l d hear right up to 18,4 khz. this is followed by a e d u c t i o n through the older age groups down to 11,15 khz γ the s e c o n d last group. the last (oldest) group constituted only one subject aged 71 years old. this subject s h o w e d a repeatable highest audible ebc frequency of 16,4 khz. figure 2 shows the mean, standard deviation and range of highest audible ebc frequency, set out in ten year age groups, for all the subjects together, except for the youngest and oldest age groups which were excluded because of one s u b j e c t only in each of these two age groups. discussion from the results of this study it would appear that the highest audible ebc frequency decreases linearly with increasing age. this decrease cannot be ascribed to being infected by mycobacterium tuberculosis or to being previously treated with any of the four standard antituberculosis drugs (rifampicin, isoniazid, pyrazinamide and e t h a m b u t o l ) as neither has any effect on hearing (tteale, goldman & pearson, 1994). if it is assumed that this is a result of sensory presbycusis due to hair cell loss in the basal cochlea (schuknecht, 1955), then it would appear that presbycusis possibly starts at a very early age and follows a distinct linear pattern of reduction in highest frequency hearing with increasing age. this phenomenon appears to affect males and females of all age groups in exactly the same way, as no statistically different gender discrepancies could be established. this differs from the findings of stelmachowicz et al. (1989) who found a small but statistically significant difference, males showing a 4,4db poorer threshold. this may be ascribed to differences in type of auditory stimulation (air conduction vs. bone conduction) used in these two studies. the finding of a gradual decrease in highest audible frequency with increasing age is in agreement with the finding of stelmachowicz et al. (1989) of a monotonical increase in high frequency threshold as a function of age. as a direct result of this linear pattern it would be a fairly easy matter to establish age-related norms for highest frequency ebc hearing. a complicating factor was the wide variation in highest audible frequency within each age group, which resulted in the normative curve being quite wide on both sides of the mean threshold line, as figure 1: mean values of highest audible ebg frequencies. table 1: statistical data by age group. figure 2: mean, stand, dev. and range of highest audible ebc. j highest audible ebc frequency (khz) ten-year age group η median mean st. dev. range ten-year age group η median mean st. dev. min. max. 1 1-10 yr 1 18,40 18,40 * 18,40 18,40 2 11-20 yr 9 16,60 16,67 1,17 15,50 18,20 3 21-30 yr 50 16,00 15,78 2,23 13,55 19,40 4 31-40 yr 42 14,50 14,21 1,84 12,37 17,40 5 41-50 yr 34 13,60 12,90 1,88 11,02 17,20 6 51-60 yr 9 11,00 11,36 1,71 9,65 13,80 7 61-70 yr 4 11,00 11,15 1,04 10,11 12,40 8 71-80 yr h 16,40 16,40 * 16,40 16,40 die suid -afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 g r voogt and η s schoeman depicted in fig. 2. despite this drawback, the data would result in a more clearly defined and perhaps more usable tentative norm for clinical use. follow-up studies utilizing much larger groups of subjects may result in the formulation of much tighter norms. until then, using a baseline ebc audiogram against which any later audiograms can be compared, seems to be the better option. the megor advantage appears to be that this method of testing, i.e., determining only the highest audible ebc frequency, is a quick and easy one to apply, as only one parameter, that of highest audible frequency at maximum stimulation intensity, needs to be examined in each patient. there could therefore be the opportunity for excellent applications in high frequency hearing screening and quick monitoring of the early effects of ototoxic drugs and perhaps also the early detection of noise induced hearing loss. the major obstacle still remains the fact that ebc thresholds reflect the hearing of both ears simultaneously, as there is as yet no reliable method of masking available on ebc audiometers. in ototoxicity monitoring, however, it would be a lesser problem as ototoxic drugs usually affect both ears equally and simultaneously. conclusion we have managed to establish some kind of tentative norms for age-related bilateral highest frequency ebc hearing. it is also suggested that this quick and easy method of measuring the highest audible ebc frequency be used for screening purposes and possibly also for ototoxic and noise damage monitoring. a larger study on a representative group of subjects may provide a much tighter set of norms and if a reliable method of masking ebc could be found, clinical norms for unilateral highest audible ebc hearing could then also be established. until then, using a baseline ebc audiogram against which any later audiograms can be compared, seems to be the better option for clinical use. references corliss, l.m., doster, m.e., simonton, j. & downs, m.p. (1970). high frequency and regular audiometry among selected groups of high school students. j. sch. health, 40, 400-404. dadson, r.s. & king, j.h. (1952). a determination of the normal threshold of hearing and its relation to the standardization of audiometers. j. laryngol. otol., 66, 366-378. fletcher, h. (1929). speech and hearing. van nostrand, new york. fletcher, j.l., cairns, a.b., collins, f.g. & endicott, j. (1967). high frequency hearing following meningitis. j. aud. res., 7,223-227. frank, t. (1990). high-frequency hearing thresholds in young adults using a commercially available audiometer. ear hear, 11(8), 450454. frank, t. & dreisbach, l.e. (1991). repeatability of high frequency thresholds. ear hear, 12(4), 294-295. galton, f. (1876). whistles for determining the upper limit of audible sound in different persons. south kensington museum conferences. harris, j.d. & myers, c.k. (1971). tentative audiometric threshold levels standards from 8 to 18 khz. j. acoust. soc. am., 49, 600601. jacobson, e. j., downs, m.p. & fletcher, j.l. (1969). clinical findings in high frequency thresholds during known ototoxic drug usage. j. aud. res., 9, 379-385. northern, j.l., downs, m.p., rudmose, w., glorig, a. & fletcher, j.l. (1972). recommended high-frequency audiometric threshold levels. j. acoust. soc. am., 52(2), 585-595. okstad, s., laukli, e. & mair, i.w.s. (1988). high frequency audiometry: comparison of electric bone-conduction and airconduction thresholds. audiology, 27,17-26. rosen, s. & olin p. (1968). hearing loss and coronary heart disease. arch. otolaryngol., 88, 251-253. rudmose, w. (1961). the suprahigh-frequency audiometer. tracor, incorporated, austin, itexas. schechter, a.m., fausti, s.a., rappaport, b.z. & frey, r.h. (1986). age categorization of high-frequency auditory threshold data. j. acoust. soc. am., 79(3), 767-771. schuknecht, h.f. (1955). presbycusis. laryngoscope, 65, 402-119. sivian, l.j. & white, s.d. (1933). on minimal audible sound fields. j. acoust. soc. am., 44, 257. stelmachowicz, p.g., beauchaine, k.a., kalberer, a. & jesteadt, w. (1989). normative thresholds in the 8to 20-khz range as a function of age. j. acoust. soc. am., 86(4), 1384-1391. iteale, c., goldman, j.m. & pearson, s.b. (1994). the association of age with the presentation and outcome of tuberculosis: a fiveyear survey. j.ar.d., 6(4), 7-10. ibnndorf, j. & kurman, b. (1984). high frequency audiometry. ann. otol. rhinol. laryngol, 93, 576-582. zislis, τ & fletcher, j.l. (1966). relation of high frequency thresholds to age and sex. j. aud. res., 6, 189-198. / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sajcd 51 speech-language assessment in a linguistically diverse setting: preliminary exploration of the possible impact of informal ‘solutions’ within the south african context j barratt, k khoza-shangase, k msimang   department of speech pathology and audiology, school of human and community development, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg joanne barratt katijah khoza-shangase kwandinjabulo msimang corresponding author: j barratt (joanne.barratt@wits.ac.za) speech-language therapists (slts) working in the context of cultural and linguistic diversity face considerable challenges in providing equitable services to all clients. this is complicated by the fact that the majority of slts in south africa are english or afrikaans speakers, while the majority of the population have a home language other than english/afrikaans. consequently, slts are often forced to call on untrained personnel to act as interpreters or translators, and to utilise informally translated materials in the assessment and management of clients with communication impairments. however, variations in translation have the potential to considerably alter intervention plans. this study explored whether the linguistic complexity conveyed in translation of the western aphasia battery (wab) test changed when translated from english to isizulu by five different first-language isizulu speakers. a qualitative comparative research design was adopted and results were analysed using comparative data analysis. results revealed notable differences in the translations, with most differences relating to vocabulary and semantics. this finding holds clinical implications for the use of informal translators as well as for the utilisation of translated material in the provision of speech-language therapy services in multilingual contexts. this study highlights the need for cautious use of translators and/or translated materials that are not appropriately and systematically adapted for local usage. further recommendations include a call for intensified efforts in the transformation of the profession within the country, specifically by attracting greater numbers of students who are fluent in african languages. keywords: interpretation, linguistic diversity, speech-language therapy, translation, western aphasia battery s afr j cd 2012;59(1):34-44. doi:10.7196/sajcd.51 the south african healthcare system is plagued with challenges, including but not limited to shortages of skills and equipment and staff retention difficulties. over and above these, there is the challenge of providing an efficient and equitable service to patients from diverse cultural and linguistic backgrounds (mosdell, balchin & ameen, 2010). the issue of cultural and linguistic diversity poses considerable challenges in the provision of speech-language therapist (slt) services since the majority of graduates are english or afrikaans speakers with little or no knowledge of other official languages and/or cultural backgrounds of their patients (gerber, 2009; bornman, sevcik, romski & pae, 2010). although english is used as the language of instruction in the majority of schools, tertiary institutions, the work place, and in healthcare settings, 92% of the total south african population are reported to have a home language other than english (statistics south africa, 2003). isizulu, the language of investigation in the current study, is reported as the mother tongue of 23.8% of the south african population. language rights of all south african citizens are firmly entrenched in, inter alia, sections 6, 29, 30 and 31 of the constitution of the republic of south africa, act 108 of 1996. given the south african government’s attempts to address language rights through changes in national policies, a higher emphasis needs to be placed on the development of culturally and linguistically suitable and reliable speech and language assessment tools (khoza, ramma, mophosho & moroka, 2008; bornman et al., 2010) with equal focus on the use of interpreters in slt engagements. this need is substantiated by research which illustrates that cross-linguistic healthcare consultations have the potential to affect the efficacy and appropriateness of intervention (swedley, stith & nelson, 2003) as well as patient satisfaction (foley, 2005). despite some preliminary attempts to translate assessment batteries into local languages (e.g. mosdell et al., 2010), difficulties continue to be incurred as a result of the range of languages spoken in south africa. consequently, one of the proposed ‘solutions’ to mitigating the negative effects of language barriers in cross-linguistic consultations has been to employ trained translators or interpreters to act as mediators. the differences between translation and interpretation may be subtle, yet may have far-reaching effects on the consultation. spiggle (1994) states that translation refers to either written or verbal communication in the second language having the same meaning when written or spoken in the first language, while interpretation relates to a deduction of information, and may refer to assessing intentions and inferences, making sense of experience and behaviour, and seeing or understanding a phenomenon in one’s own terms by grasping its essence (spiggle, 1994). many researchers prefer the use of the term adaptation as opposed to translation or interpretation as there is an acknowledgement that there is a requirement for adaptation in culture, content and wording, over and above the simple translation of a test (geisinger, 1994). davidson (2001) claims that interpretation is always contextual; however in institutional settings, such as hospitals, interpretation may be defined by social and behavioural norms. davidson (2001) asserts that in the medical setting, clinicians usually judge the patients’ physical and verbal signs for social and moral relevance in order to make a diagnosis, and the current authors argue that an untrained interpreter may not look for such cues during a mediated consultation. moreover, in speech-language assessments, untrained interpreters may not be aware that subtle changes or inaccuracies in understanding or production can indicate an impairment that requires intervention. adaptation of test materials compounds this issue to a further degree. despite the fact that south african legislation, as indicated by the policy on language services (2011) for the department of health (department of health, 2011), promotes the use of interpreters in healthcare settings, few trained interpreters are available in the public healthcare sector (penn, 2007). as a result, most therapists rely on untrained interpreters and translators during assessments and therapy sessions. in hospital settings, nurses, cleaners, general assistants, and family members are frequently called upon to translate or interpret for patients who do not speak and/or understand english. this observation is confirmed by elderkin-thompson, silver and waitzkin (2001), who report that bilingual individuals with no formal training as translators frequently translate for patients who are non-english speaking. much meaningful information may be lost since the person translating may not have an understanding of terms used, may use the notion of direct translation or translate the message differently, thus creating confusion and changing the meaning of the item to be translated (elderkin-thompson et al., 2001). previous research has also shown that ad hoc interpreters can commit many errors of interpretation such as omitting important information as a result of limited vocabulary during translation (flores, laws, mayo, zuckerman, abreu, medina & hardt, 2003). it can thus be anticipated that results obtained from mediated slt assessments may be compromised, leading to incorrect diagnoses, particularly where untrained translators or interpreters are used. in speech-language therapy this challenge is more complex because how communication occurs is a crucial aspect, not just what is communicated as in most other healthcare setting scenarios. owing to the limited focused research on this challenge within the south african context, the ad hoc arrangements made during assessment and management of clients from linguistically and culturally diverse populations creates translation and adaptation difficulties which arguably influence the normative interpretation of assessment measures, and consequently management plans. despite the availability of frameworks and guidelines for adapting or translating test measures (e.g. geisinger, 1994), as well as published guidelines for working with interpreters, anecdotal evidence suggests that these are infrequently followed by slts in the south african context; this is believed to have a negative impact on speech-language therapy service provision as a whole. one of the populations with whom slts commonly work is the neurologically impaired adult population. van schoor, van niekerk and grobbelaar (2001) reported that south africa is rated among the highest in the world for casualty admission rates secondary to motor vehicle accidents. global statistics reveal that south africa has among the highest hiv/aids infection rates in the world, with an estimated 410 000 new infections in 2010 (statistics south africa, 2010). mochan, modi and modi (2003) show a relationship between hiv infection and cerebrovascular accidents as a result of vascular abnormalities, coagulation disorders and cardio-embolic disease. this high incidence of stroke concurs with the findings of the southern africa stroke prevalence initiative (saspi) project team (2004), who report an incidence of cerebrovascular accidents of 243 per 100 000 people. as a result of the above-mentioned factors, it can be assumed that in south africa the number of patients with speech, language and/or cognitive-linguistic impairments as a consequence of neurological impairment is high, resulting in an increased need for speech-language therapy assessment and intervention. one commonly used formal english assessment tool for speech-language and cognitive-linguistic assessment of the neurologically impaired adult population is the western aphasia battery (wab) (kertesz, 1982). this tool includes subtests relating to content, fluency, auditory comprehension, repetition, naming, reading, writing and calculation (kertesz, 1982), and thus assesses areas of functioning in which patients with traumatic brain injuries also experience difficulty (kennedy & yorkston, 2000). formal assessment measures provide a baseline for intervention, which may be compromised when a patient’s first language is not english. this becomes abundantly clear in a multilingual context like south africa where the majority of formal assessment measures available are written in english and standardised in different settings. as a result, ad hoc translators are often used in clinical encounters, and this may influence the assessment findings in a number of ways. the untrained individual’s lack of familiarity with the speech materials presented and compromised competence with the language that the clinician speaks may influence the accuracy of translation. translators who are neither competent in english nor the language being translated may not perform as well as native speakers of that language. hence, slts need to take cognisance of these factors and how their lack of knowledge of the native languages of their clients can negatively affect the accuracy of assessment results, preventing them from fully meeting their clients’ needs. this heightened awareness should range from case history taking to the actual assessment to the test material used in the assessment, and therefore more emphasis should to be placed on development of assessment and intervention materials in all languages. in response to this need within the south african slt profession, the current study explored whether differences existed in the translation of the wab by five first-language isizulu speakers. in a multilingual context, it is well documented that many of the formal speech-language assessments are inappropriate for first-language english speakers, and indeed speakers of other languages. it is therefore crucial for all avenues to be explored in order to appropriately adapt or develop the most accurate and efficient assessment tools for the african continent. methodology prior to the study being conducted, permission was obtained from the university of the witwatersrand non-medical ethics committee (protocol number: h090404). aim to determine whether the linguistic complexity of the test items of the wab changed when translated from english to isizulu. objective to determine whether differences existed in the translation of the wab from english to isizulu when translated by five first-language isizulu speakers. research design a comparative qualitative research design was employed in order to determine whether similarities and differences existed within the data rather than starting with predefined concepts (ovretveit, 1998). data collection the initial part of the study involved translating the wab from english to isizulu in order to investigate whether the syntax and semantics as well as item complexity and familiarity changed during translation. this translation was performed by the third author (km – translator 1). another translation was then performed by a senior lecturer from the department of african languages (all – translator 2). the two sets of translations were compared and one set was agreed upon for use as a baseline comparative set. for the purpose of this study, the translation made by km was chosen to be the baseline for comparison. this choice was based on the fact that she had detailed knowledge on the test material being used for translation, as well as the purpose for which the assessment tool was established. furthermore, while the translations made by both translator 1 and translator 2 were considered to be accurate, translator 1’s choice of words was thought to be less academic/formal, and therefore more familiar to the participants. once the wab had been translated, the three subtests deemed by the researchers to comprise the most complex linguistic features were identified: the subtests of auditory verbal comprehension, sequential commands and spontaneous speech. these were then utilised in the second phase of the study where verbal translations were conducted with the five participants (tables 2 4). data collection took place with each participant individually in a quiet private consulting room at the university speech and hearing clinic. the researcher dictated each item of each of the subtests individually, and the participants were instructed to verbally translate these from english to isizulu. these translations were digitally audio-recorded and later transcribed. participants five first-language isizulu speakers were recruited using convenience sampling. the choice of isizulu speakers was based on the fact that this is the most widely spoken african language in south africa (stats sa, 2003), and represents the most commonly spoken african language at the university speech and hearing clinic. furthermore, both the second and third authors are first-language isizulu speakers. inclusion criteria stipulated that all participants needed to be over the age of 18 years, be first-language isizulu speakers, and should consider themselves to be proficient in english as per self-report. table 1 provides a description of the participants. all participants were provided with information letters and consent forms in both english and isizulu prior to the commencement of data collection. where the participants were unable to read, the researcher verbally explained the study to them in isizulu and verbal assent was accepted in lieu of written consent. ethical considerations were guided by the principles of the south african medical research council (south african medical research council, 2003). table 1. personal characteristics of the participants participant age gender occupation highest level of education languages spoken place of origin/birth translator 1 22 female speech and hearing therapist ba speech and hearing therapy isizulu, english, afrikaans newcastle translator 2 38 female senior lecturer phd (african languages) isizulu, english johannesburg 1 56 male gardener grade 11 isizulu and english rural kwazulu-natal 2 52 female nursing sister diploma in nursing isizulu and english pietermaritzburg 3 23 female student 3 years at university studying law isizulu and english pietermaritzburg 4 38 female cleaner grade 9 isizulu, english and sotho newcastle 5 34 male painter & fixes air conditioners grade 12 isizulu, english and sotho soweto data analysis and trustworthiness of findings data were analysed qualitatively using comparative analysis. each translation was transcribed and back-translated from isizulu to english. thereafter, each translation was compared against the baseline translation. furthermore the translations were compared with each other so as to identify differences in translation across the five participants. trustworthiness of findings was accounted for by means of back-translation of the baseline wab, as well as independent validity checks of two of the five participants’ translations to eliminate bias. results results are presented in accordance with the aims of the study. results of the translations of the most linguistically complex subtests of the wab are depicted in table 2 (tables 2 4 are placed at the end of the article to facilitate the flow of the article). comparison of translations made by the researcher and african languages lecturer (all) initially, the translations made by the researcher and the all were compared. while some differences relating to sentence structure and vocabulary were noted, these appeared to be mainly dialectal in nature and did not affect the overall linguistic complexity of the test items. the reasons behind these differences appeared to pertain to the all’s more formal and academic use of isizulu, while the researcher’s choice of words was more colloquial. within the subtest of auditory verbal comprehension, a few changes to structure and complexity of the questions were noted. an example of this can be seen in the translation of the question ‘is the door closed?’ in english this phrase consists of the structure copula determiner noun adjective, whereas in isizulu the structure changes to adjective noun which is translated to ‘uvaliwe umnyango?’ although the structure of the question changes, the meaning remains the same, and when translated back into english the question stays the same. similarly, by translating the question ‘are you wearing red pyjamas?’ to isizulu (‘ugqoke izimpahla zokulala ezibomvu?’) the sentence structure changes to ‘are you wearing pyjamas that are red?’ this indicates that the meaning does not change, although the length of the question increases. this was also noted in the phrase, ‘do you eat a banana before you peel it?’(‘uhluba ubhanana ngaphampi kokuba uwudle?’). in this phrase the noun phrase (np) is followed by the verb phrase (vp) in english. however, in isizulu the vp is followed by the np, further indicating that the structure of the question changes, yet does not compromise the meaning of the question. when translated back to english, the phrase reads as ‘do you peel a banana before you eat it?’ in the subtest of spontaneous speech, differences were also noted. the researcher translated the phrase ‘what is your occupation?’ to ‘usebenza laphi?’ which, when translated back into english means ‘where do you work?’ this implies that the response obtained could refer to place of occupation but not the type of occupation. some differences were also noted in the translation of the auditory verbal comprehension subtest. for example, the researcher translated the command ‘shut your eyes’ as ‘vala amehlo’ whereas the all translated it as ‘chimeza’. both of these translations are accurate representations of the english command as a patient would perform the command correctly if translated either way. the only important difference in the two translations is that the translation made by the researcher can be back-translated into english as ‘close your eyes’ whereas the translation made by the all would be translated to english as ‘shut your eyes’. another difference was noted in the command ‘point to the chair’. people from the more rural parts of kwazulu-natal and those who have formally studied isizulu tend to use the word ‘isihlalo’ as used by the all, for ‘chair’. however, the researcher translated the word ‘chair’ as ‘isitulo’. the word ‘isitulo’ is a ‘borrowed word’ taken from the afrikaans word ‘stoel’ and is used more commonly in more urban areas, whereas ‘isihlalo’ is the more authentic version of the word ‘chair’ in isizulu. in some instances, the word order of the sequential commands changed. an example of this is seen in the command ‘point to the comb with the pen’, which was translated to ‘ngepeni, khomba ikama’, meaning ‘with the pen, point to the comb’. although this change did not compromise the overall meaning of the command, it has the potential to affect a patient’s understanding of the command and may also change the sequential order in which they carry out more complex commands. most of the changes that were noted from the initial translation of the wab were concerned with structure and vocabulary. since the all had studied isizulu at both an undergraduate and postgraduate level, her translations were more formal as opposed to the more colloquial translations by the researcher. this implies that level of education may play an influential role on translation. these effects of education may also be true for the patient population. this is of significance to slts since the risk of misdiagnosis of communication impairments may be increased by failure to ensure effective translation of assessment materials or by assessing the patient in a language that he/she does not understand (stolk, ziguras, saunders, garlick, stuart & coffey, 1998). this suggests that slts should be cautious in their interpretation of assessment findings elicited through informally mediated consultations. comparison of verbal translations made by the five participants the results of this aspect of the study have been tabulated in table 2. based on these results, it was noted that many more discrepancies were noted across the translations, specifically those concerning vocabulary used, as well as sentence structure and semantics. table 2. translation of the auditory verbal comprehension subtest of the wab wab command translation by translator 1(km) and translator 2 (all) participant zulu translation english translation is your name smith? 1: igama lakho usmith?   2: igama lakho usmith? 1 igama lakho ungusmith na? is your name smith? 2 igama lakho usmith? is your name smith? 3 igama lakho usmith? is your name smith? 4 igama lakho usmith na? is your name smith? 5 igama lakho usmith? is your name smith? is your name brown? 1: igama lakho ubrown?   2: igama lakho ubrown 1 uwu mr brown? is your name brown? 2 igama lakho ubrown? is your name brown? 3 igama lakho ubrown? is your name brown? 4 igama lakho ubrown? is your name brown? 5 igama lakho ubrown? is your name brown? is your name (real name)? 1: igama lakho u (real name)?   2: igama lakho u (real name)?   1 igama lakho u(real name) na? is your name (real name)? 2 igama lakho u(real name)? is your name (real name)? 3 igama lakho u(real name)? is your name (real name)? 4 igama lakho u(real name)? is your name (real name)? 5 igama lakho u(real name)? is your name (real name)? do you live in toronto? 1: uhlala toronto?   2: uhlala toronto na? 1 uhlala etoronto? do you live in toronto? 2 uhlala etoronto? do you live in toronto? 3 uhlala etoronto? do you live in toronto? 4 uhlala etoronto na? do you live in toronto? 5 uhlala etoronto? do you live in toronto? do you live in windsor? 1: uhlala ewindsor?   2: uhlala ewindsor na? 1 uhlala ewindsor? do you live in windsor? 2 uhlala ewindsor? do you live in windsor? 3 uhlala ewindsor? do you live in windsor? 4 uhlala ewindsor? do you live in windsor? 5 uhlala ewindsor? do you live in windsor? wab command translation by translator 1(km) and translator 2 (all) participant zulu translation english translation are you a man or a woman? 1: uyindoda noma umuntu wesifazane?   2: uyindoda noma umuntu wesifazane? 1 umuntu wesilisa noma wesifazane? are you a male or female? 2 uyindoda noma umuntu wesifazane? are you a man or a woman? 3 uyinkosikazi noma indoda are you a woman or a man? 4 ungumuntu wesifazane noma umuntu wesilisa? are you a female or a male? 5 ingabe uyindoda noma umfazi? could you be a man or a woman? are you a doctor? 1: ungudokotela?   2: ungudokotela na? 1 ungudokotela? are you a doctor? 2 ungudokotela? are you a doctor? 3 uwudokotela yini? are you a doctor? 4 ungu-doctor are you a doctor? 5 igabe uwudokotela? could you be a doctor? am i a man or a woman? 1: ngiwumuntu wesifazane noma wesilisa?   2: ngiyindoda noma umuntu wesifazane? 1 nguye umuntu wesifazane? is she the female here? 2 mina ngowesifazane? am i a woman? 3 lo, uyindoda noma wumuntu wesifazane? is she a male or female? 4 ungumuntu wesifazane noma wesilisa? are you a man or a woman? 5 ngigabe ngiyindoda noma umfazi? am i a man or a woman? are the lights on in this room? 1: ugesi uyakhanya yini la-endlini?   2: ugesi uyakhanya kulelikamelo na?   1 kukhanyiswe la endlini? are the lights on in this room? 2 uyakhanya ugesi kuleligumbi? is the light on in this room? 3 kuyakhanya kulendlu noma akukhanyi? are the lights on in this room or not? 4 i-light ikhanyisiwe kulendlu? are the lights on in this room? 5 ama-light ngabe ayakhanya kuleli-room? are the lights on in this room? is the door closed? 1: umnyango uvaliwe?   2: uvaliwe yini umnyango? 1 umnyango uvaliwe? is the door closed? 2 umnyango uvaliwe? is the door closed? 3 umnyango uvaliwe? is the door closed? 4 umnyango uvaliwe? is the door closed? 5 umnyango uvaliwe? is the door closed? is this a hotel? 1: isehotela la?   2: isehotela lapha? 1 ihotela leli? is this a hotel? 2 ihotela leli? is this a hotel? 3 kusehotela la yini? is this a hotel? 4 ihotela leli? is this a hotel? 5 ihotela leli? is this a hotel? is this a therapy room? 1: i-therapy room yini le?   2: igumbi lokuhlolela leli? 1 indawo yokuhlolela le? is this an assessment room? 2 i-therapy room le? is this a therapy room? 3 kuse-therapy room yini la? is this a therapy room? 4 lena i-therapy room? is this a therapy room? 5 igabe igumbi lokongela leli? could this be an assessment room? are you wearing red pyjamas? 1: ugqoke amapyjama abomvu yini?   2: ugqoke izimpahla zokulala ezibomvu? 1 ugqoke ama-pyjama abovu? are you wearing red pyjamas? 2 ugqoke ama-pyjama abovu na? are you wearing red pyjamas? 3 ugqoke ama-pyjama abovu yini? are you wearing red pyjamas? 4 ugqoke i-pyjama lokulala elibovu? are you wearing sleeping pyjamas that are red? 5 ugqoke ama-pyjama abovu? are you wearing red pyjamas? will paper burn in fire? 1: iphpha lingasha emlilweni?   2: iphepha lingasha yini emlilweni? 1 iphepha lingasha emlilweni? can paper burn in fire? 2 iphepha liyasha yini emlilweni? does paper burn in fire? 3 amaphepha ayasha yini emlilweni? can papers burn in fire? 4 iphepha liyavutha na? is paper burning? 5 igabe iphepha liyasha emlilweni? can paper burn in fire? wab command translation by translator 1(km) and translator 2 (all) participant zulu translation english translation does march come before june? 1: inynaga ka-march ifika kuqala kuneya-june?   2: inynaga ka-march ifika kuqala kuneya-june?   1 kuyenzeka lenyanga ka-march ifike ngaphambi kwa-june? does it happen that the month of march comes before june? 2 u-march ufika ngaphambi kwa-june? does march come before june? 3 yikuphi okuqamuka phambili, u-march noma u-june? what comes first march or june? 4 inyanga ka-march ifika kuqala kuno-june na? does the month of march come before june? 5 igabe u-march ufika before u-june? could march come before june? do you eat a banana before you peel it? 1: uyawuhluba kuqala yini ubanana ngaphambi wokuthi uwudle?   2: uyawuhluba kuqala yini ubanana ngaphambi wokuthi uwudle? 1 uyawudla ubanana ungakawuhlubi? do you eat a banana before you’ve peeled it? 2 ubanana uwudla ungaka wuhlubi na? do you eat a banana before you peel it? 3 udla ubanana ngoba usuwuhlubile yini? do you eat a banana because you have peeled it? 4 udla ubanana kuqala before uwuhluba? do you eat a banana first before you peel it? 5 igabe uyalidla ibanana ngaphambi wokuthi ulihlubile? can you eat a banana before you peel it? does it snow in july? 1: liyakhithika yini ngo-july?   2: liyakhithika yinii ngo-july? 1 kuyenzeka likhithike ngo-july? can it snow in july? 2 liyakhithika ngo-july? does it snow in july? 3 kuyakhithika yini ngo-july? does it snow in july? 4 liyakhithika ngo-july? does it snow in july? 5 liyasnowa na ku-july? does it snow in july? is a horse larger than a dog? 1: ihhashi likhulu kunenja?   2: ihhashi likhulu yini kunenja? 1 kuyenzeka ihhashi libel’khulu kunenja? can a horse be bigger than a dog? 2 ihhashi likhulu kunenja yini? is a horse bigger than a dog? 3 yini enkulu, inja noma ihhashi? what is larger, a dog or a horse? 4 ihhashi likhulu kunenja yini? is a horse larger than a dog? 5 igabe ihhashi likhulu na kunenja? is a horse larger than a dog? do you cut the grass with an axe? 1: uyabusika yini utshani ngembazo?   2: uyabusika yini utshani ngembazo? 1 uyakwazi ukuthi ngembazo ugence utshani? can you cut grass with an axe? 2 utshanii ubugunda ngembazo yini? do you cut grass with an axe? 3 uma usika utshani, usika ngembazo na? when you cut grass, do you use an axe? 4 ugangamula utshani ngombese? can you cut grass with a knife? 5 igabe usika utshani ngembazo? do you cut the grass with an axe? variations in translation relating to vocabulary as evident in table 2, much of the vocabulary used in the subtest may be considered inappropriate for assessment in south africa. this concurs with the findings of mosdell et al. (2010), who also found that test items in the boston naming test and the cookie theft test were both linguistically and culturally biased, affecting the reliability of the tests when used in the south african context. over and above the effect that culturally inappropriate vocabulary may have on the responses obtained from a patient, the nature of the vocabulary may be altered by the translator if he/she has limited vocabulary in his/her own language and/or in english. for example, when looking at nouns such as smith, brown, windsor and toronto, it may not be easy for a second-language speaker of english to relate to such words, and may thus affect the results of the testing procedure. anderson (1992) recommends that materials used during assessments and therapy should be culturally and linguistically sensitive so as to appropriately guide patient management. vocabulary such as ‘hotel’ and ‘snow’ in the wab may be inappropriate to use in a country such as south africa, especially in the government healthcare context, since many people in this context may not be familiar with the concept of a hotel. similarly, many people in south africa have never seen snow before or heard what it is called in languages other than their own. for this reason it may be difficult to comment on when it snows because of the fact that they do not have any experience on which to base this. the wab was standardised in new york and therefore the expected response for the absurdity question ‘does it snow in july?’ is ‘no’. however, in south africa it is quite possible to have snow in certain parts of the country at this time of year. this is therefore clinically relevant as a patient may then be thought to be presenting with impaired comprehension because of the fact that he answered the question incorrectly. the word ‘axe’ is also considered inappropriate for use in a south african assessment battery. this is due to the fact that some patients may be unfamiliar with the name of this object and therefore may have difficulty in answering the question. for example, participant 4 translated ‘do you cut grass with an axe?’ as ‘unganqamula utshani ngombese?’ meaning ‘can you cut grass with a knife?’ this may be due to the fact that she does not have the vocabulary to make a connection between the english word ‘axe’ and the isizulu word ‘imbazo’. implications of this mistranslation which leads to changed meaning entirely are considerable and have direct impact on test scoring as well as eventual diagnosis. errors in translation of syntax and/or semantics word order is an aspect that may be altered during translation and may have negative effects on slt assessments. berndt (2001) states that verbal short-term memory is frequently affected in patients with neurological impairments. therefore a change in sentence structure may have a negative effect on the neurologically impaired patient’s ability to comprehend an instruction. from the subtest of auditory verbal comprehension (table 2), a number of semantic differences in translation were apparent. participant 3 translated the question ‘are the lights on in this room?’ as ‘are the lights on in this room or not?’ similarly, participant 3 changed the nature of the question ‘does march come before june?’ to ‘what comes first, march or june?’ and translated ‘is a horse larger than a dog?’ to ‘what is larger, a dog or a horse?’ in all of these instances, patients with neurological impairments may have difficulty in responding accurately since the nature of the question has been changed from one which requires a yes/no response to a forced alternative, and the processing requirements being assessed have also been simplified. in the questions ‘are you a man or a woman?’, ‘are you a doctor?’ and ‘is this an assessment room?’, participant 5 consistently asked the question by saying ‘could you/this be a woman/doctor/therapy room?’ this increases the level of complexity of the question and requires a degree of reasoning in formulating an answer, which may be difficult for the neurologically impaired patient, or the patient’s reasoning may be difficult for the slt to follow. in the question ‘am i a man or a woman?’ participants 1, 2, 3 and 4 all changed the nature of the semantics, thus posing the questions ‘is she the female here?’, ‘am i a woman?’, ‘is she a male or a female?’ and ‘are you a man or a woman?’ respectively. another change in semantics can be seen in the question ‘will paper burn in a fire?’ where participant 4 translated it to ‘is paper burning?’, thereby altering the meaning conveyed in the question and the expected response. these examples not only reveal the impact on complexity of the stimuli, but also highlight the influence of informal translation on validity of the instrument. the only semantic differences in the spontaneous speech subtest (table 3) were in the question ‘what is your occupation?’ this may be because few people use the term ‘occupation’ when they speak about work, but tend to use the word ‘job’ or ‘work’, and may explain participant 2’s translation to ‘where do you work?’ which resulted in a change of semantics. in the same question, participant 4 mistook the meaning of the word ‘occupation’ for the meaning of ‘address’ in the question ‘what is your occupation?’, thereby completely altering the meaning of the question. table 3. translation of the spontaneous speech subtest of the wab wab question translation by translator 1(km) and translator 2 (all) participant zulu translation english translation how are you today? 1: unjani namhlanje?   2: unjani namhlanje? 1 unjani namhlanje? how are you today? 2 unjani namhlanje? how are you today? 3 unjani namhlanje? how are you today? 4 unjani namhlanje? how are you today? 5 unjani namhlanje? how are you today? have you been here before? 1: usuke weza lapha ngaphambilini?   2: usuke weza lapha ngaphambilini? 1 ukewaba-la? have you been here before? 2 waka wafika-la phambili? have you been here before? 3 wakeweza-la ngelinye ilanga? have you been here previously? 4 ukewaba-la phambilini? have you been here before? 5 usuke waza la-phambilini? have you been here before? what is your name? 1: ubani igama lakho?   2: ubani igama lakho? 1 igama lakho ungubani? what is your name? 2 ubani igama lakho? what is your name? 3 ubani igama lakho? what is your name? 4 ubani igama lakho? what is your name? 5 ubani igama lakho? what is your name? what is your address? 1: lithini ikheli lakho?   2: lithini ikheli lakho? 1 ikheli lakho lithini? what is your address? 2 lithini ikheli lakho? what is your address? 3 lithini ikheli lakho? what is your address? 4 ithini i-address yakho? what is your address? 5 la ohlalakhona, i-address where you stay, what is your address? what is your occupation? 1: usebenza laphi?   2: usebenza msebenzi muni? 1 usebenza msebenzi muni? what is your occupation? 2 usebenza kuphi? where do you work? 3 wenza msebenzi muni? what is your occupation? 4 ilokunjana yakho…. address? participant thought occupation was address, therefore unable to translate the question. 5 igabe uwenza msebenzi muni? what is your occupation? tell me a little about why you are here? 1: ngitshele kabanzi ukuthi yini ekubeka la-namhlanje.   2: ngitshele kabanzi ukuthi yini ekubeka la-namhlanje.   1 awuthi ukungitshela fahla ukuthi ubekwayini la. tell me briefly why you are here. 2 ongitshele kancane ukuthi ufunani la. please tell me a little about why you are here today. 3 awungitshele ukuthi namhlanje uzeleni la? please can you please tell me why you are here today? 4 ngitshele kancane ukuthi yini uze-la namhlanje. tell me a little about why you came here today. 5 ngitshele kabanzi ukuthi yini ula-namhlanje. tell me a little why you are here. analysis of the subtest of sequential commands (table 4) revealed that in the command ‘point with the pen to the book’ participants 2 and 3 changed the word order to ‘khomba ibuku ngepeni’ and ‘khomba incwadi ngepeni’ respectively, meaning ‘point to the book with the pen’, thus changing the sequential nature of the command. furthermore, participant 4 translated the same command to ‘khomba ikamu ne-ball peni’ meaning ‘point to the comb with the pen’ thus changing the semantics of the command. similarly, in the command ‘point to the window then to the door’, participants 1 and 4 both translated the command as ‘point to the window and the door’, and in the command ‘point to the pen and the book’, participants 3 and 5 both translated the command to ‘point to the pen and then point to the book.’ these errors in translation affect the expected sequential order of the response and consequently patients may be misdiagnosed as having auditory memory impairments and/or sequencing problems incorrectly. in the command ‘point to the comb with the pen’, participants 1 and 4 translated this as ‘point with the pen to the comb’ which, although it maintains the semantics of the command, changes the complexity. similarly, the command ‘with the book, point to the comb’ yielded errors in translation, where participant 2 translated it as ‘point with the comb to the book’, thus changing the semantics, and participant 4 translated it as ‘on the book point to the book’ thus providing a nonsensical command. the command ‘put the comb on the other side of the pen and turn over the book’ was problematic for almost all of the participants because of the increasing complexity of the command, with only participant 5 giving an accurate translation of the command. an important factor highlighted by berndt (2001) is that most neurologically impaired patients have impairments related to sentence comprehension tasks, especially with semantically reversible sentences that are syntactically complex. this implies that a patient who already presents with a neurological impairment may have marked difficulties in answering a question with complex semantics and syntax, especially where translation is necessary; where inaccurate translations are performed, the assessment process is therefore severely hampered. table 4. translation of the sequential commands subtest of the wab wab command translation by translator 1(km) and translator 2 (all) participant zulu translation english translation raise your hands 1: phakamisa izandla zakho   2: phakamisa izandla zakho 1 phakamisa isandla raise your hand 2 ngicela uphakamise izandla please raise your hands 3 phakamisa izandla zakho raise your hands 4 phakamisa izandla zakho raise your hands 5 phakamisa isandla sakho raise your hand shut your eyes 1: vala amehlo   2: chimeza 1 vala amehlo close your eyes 2 chimeza amehlo shut your eyes 3 vala amehlo akho close your eyes 4 vala amehlo close your eyes 5 vala amehlo akho close your eyes point to the chair 1: khomba isitulo   2: khimba isihlalo 1 khomba isitulo/isihlalo point to the chair 2 khomba isitulo point to the chair 3 khomba isitulo point to the chair 4 khomba isitulo point to the chair 5 khomba isitulo point to the chair point to the window, then to the door 1: khomba ifasitela bese ukhomba umnyango   2: khomba ifasitela bese ukhomba umnyango   1 khomba efasteleni nase mnyango point to the window and the door 2 khomba ifasitela mese ukhomba umnyango point to the window and then to the door 3 khomba ifasitela bese ukhomba umnyango point to the window and then to the door 4 khomba ifasitela nomnyango point to the window and the door 5 khomba ifasitela bese ukhomba umnynago point to the window and then point to the door point to the pen and the book 1: khomba ipeni nencwadi   2: khomba ipeni nencwadi   1 khomba ipeni nebuku point to the pen and the book 2 khomba ipeni nebuku point to the pen and the book 3 khomba ibodwe bese ukhomba ibuku point to the pot and then to the book 4 khomba i-ball peni nebuku point to the pen with the book 5 khomba ipeni bese ukhomba incwadi point to the pen and then point to the book point with the pen to the book 1: ngepeni khomba incwadi   2: khomba ngepeni encwadini 1 khomba ngepeni ebukwini point with the pen to the book 2 khomba ibuku ngepeni point to the book with the pen 3 khomba incwadi ngepeni point to the book with the pen 4 khomba ikamu ne-ball peni point to the comb and the pen 5 khomba ngepeni encwadini point with the pen to the book point to the comb with the pen 1: ngepeni khomba ikamu   2: khomba ikamu ngepeni 1 khomba ngepeni ekamini point with the pen to the comb 2 khomba ikamu ngepeni point to the comb with the pen 3 khomba ikamu ngepeni point to the comb with the pen 4 khomba ngepeni ekamini point with the pen to the comb 5 khomba ikamu ngepeni point to the comb with the pen with the book point to the comb 1: ngencwadi, khomba ikamu   2: ngencwadi, khomba ikamu 1 ngencwadi, khomba ekamini with the book point to the comb 2 khomba ikamu ngebuku point with the comb to the book 3 ngebuku khomba ikamu with the book point to the comb 4 ebukwini pointa ibuku on the book point to the book 5 ngencwadi, khomba ikamu with the book point to the comb wab command translation by translator 1(km) and translator 2 (all) participant zulu translation english translation put the pen on top of the book then give it to me 1: beka ipeni phezu kwencwadi bese unikeza mina   2: beka ipeni phezu kwencwadi bese unikeza mina   1 awuthathe ipeni ulibeke phezu ebukwini bese unikeza mina take the pen and put it on top of the book and give it to me 2 beka ipeni phezu kwebuku mese uletha la-kimina put the pen on top of the book and then give it to me 3 beka ipeni lakho phezu kwencwadi bese unikeza mina put your pen on top of the book and then give it to me 4 thatha i-ball peni ulibeke phezu kwebuku mese unikeza mina take the pen and put it on top of the book and then give it to me 5 beka ipeni phezu kwencadi bese unikeza yena put the pen on top of the book and then give it to her put the comb on the other side of the pen and turn over the book 1: beka ikamu ngale kwepeni bese uphendula incwadi   2: beka ikamu kuloluhlangothi lwepeni bese uphendula incwadi   1 thatha ikamu ulibeke enxenyeni ethile kwincwadi bese uvala ibuku take the comb and put it on the other side of the book and then close the book 2 beka ikamu kulelelinye icela lepeni mese uvala ibuku put the comb on the other side of the pen and then close the book 3 beka ipeni ngalelinye icala bese ulibeka phezu kwencwadi put the pen on the other side and put it on top of the book 4 beka i-ball peni phezu kwekamu bese ngiyayiphendula bese unikeza mina put the pen on top of the comb and then i will turn it over and give it to me 5 beka ikamu kuloluhlangothi lwencwadi bese uphendula incwadi put the comb on the other side of the book and then turn over the book discussion as illustrated in this preliminary study, numerous differences and errors in translation may occur when untrained translators are used, and these have the potential to alter the responses obtained from the patient. this may result in misdiagnosis and consequently inappropriate intervention. while it is also not possible to fully identify the characteristics that make one translator more accurate than another, there are a number of factors that can be identified from the current study. the following factors need consideration when translations are conducted from english to isizulu: the impact of isizulu as a tonal language on translation stress and intonation patterns impact substantially on the semantics of spoken language in isizulu. this implies that lexical tone can be used to attach different meanings to words which share the same phonemic content (kuun, zimu, barnard & davel, 2005). the effects of this were noted, for example, during the transcription of participant 4’s question ‘am i a man or a woman?’ which was translated to ‘ungumuntu wesifazane noma wesilisa?’ as a result of the intonation patterns of the language, stress can be placed in different places on the word ‘ungumuntu’ thereby changing the meaning of the sentence. the subtle difference between ‘ungumuntu’ (is she/he) and ‘ungumuntu’ (are you) will affect the patient’s response. this is an important consideration since the ability to accurately detect intonation is a cognitive function involving the right hemisphere (kuun et al., 2005); therefore it has significance in the assessment of a neurologically impaired individual.   the impact of gender on translation although the sample size was small, it was observed that generally the male participants translated the test items with the closest level of accuracy to the original stimuli. it is unclear why the males in this study consistently gave more accurate translations than the females; however, this finding is of interest given that in most clinical encounters, females are more likely to be requested to assist with translation when a language barrier exists. within the south african context, females dominate the nursing profession, and they are also the ones who most often take the role of caregivers. this reality is also confirmed by friedland and penn (2003), who state that it is typical within the south african healthcare sector for nurses and cleaning staff, who are predominantly female, to act as translators in cross-linguistic encounters. interestingly, on a few occasions in the current study, female participants used the carrier phrase ‘please’ during translation of test items. for example, in the spontaneous speech subtest, both participants 2 and 3 translated ‘tell me a little about why you are here’ to ‘please tell me a little about why you are here’. similarly in the sequential command ‘raise your hands’, participant 2 translated this to ‘please raise your hands’. while the sample is too small to draw definitive conclusions as to why this occurred, it is possible that the female participants, because of their ‘nurturing’ nature attempt to establish a rapport with the ‘patient’ during the process of informal translation. this use of the carrier phrase may also be related to cultural norms in isizulu which may dictate subservience on the part of females in general societal functions and engagements. the impact of education on translation based on the current findings, it was evident that level of education also appears to play a role in the type of translation. participant 4 has the lowest level of education. a number of variations were noted in the translations made by this participant compared with the other translators, with the most significant variation being in the translation of ‘what is your occupation?’ owing to the fact that participant 4 may not have had extensive exposure to formal english because of her level of education, she was unable to understand the term ‘occupation’, but instead interpreted it as ‘address’. ercikan (1998) states that a translation made by an interpreter must reflect not only the meaning of the original item, but should also maintain the same relevance, intrinsic interest and familiarity of the item content. furthermore, ercikan (1998) argues that if this is not done, what the item measures may be altered. the current authors agree with this statement as evidence from the current study provides some support for it. a further example of the impact of education on translation can be seen in the subtest of auditory verbal comprehension, where participant 4 was unable to provide the correct term for the object ‘axe’, indicating that she did not have sufficient vocabulary to make the translation. however, having said this, the participant with the highest level of education did not always translate the test items with the closest degree of accuracy. when the impact of level of education on translation was discussed with all, she revealed that ‘reasons’ for translation also play a role in how translation is done, and this may not necessarily be related to level of education. furthermore, she reported that translating items for a ‘language assessment’ may pose significant challenges for the translator and requires a higher level of precision than ‘routine’ translation. therefore, despite the fact that in the south african context english is learned at school by the majority of the population and it can be argued that level of education may give a translator access to a greater vocabulary or to a greater degree of bilingualism, it may not necessarily improve their ability to make translations without changing the inherent meaning, especially in cases where the purpose of the translation has not been explicitly described. the role of multilingualism and language exposure on translation the number of languages that a translator is able to speak appears to play a role in the type of vocabulary that is used during translation. this was observed, for example, in the subtest of auditory verbal comprehension when the question, ‘are the lights on in this room?’ was posed. participants 1, 2 and 3 translated this sentence correctly. however, participants 4 and 5 substituted the isizulu word ‘ugesi’ with the english word ‘light’. this can possibly be explained by looking into where the participants are from. participant 4 is from newcastle in kwazulu-natal, where isizulu is the dominant language, but many people from different backgrounds live there, and a number of languages are spoken. the same applies to participant 5 who is from soweto. therefore, it is understandable that they would not use the authentic isizulu term, ‘ugesi’ for ‘lights’. nevertheless, it is unlikely that this translation would negatively influence the response elicited. the use of ‘borrowed’ english words during translation may also be related to age, in that the younger participants are more likely to have had more exposure to english as a result of being born during a time when more people were educated with english as a medium of instruction, and also having had more exposure to english on television. conclusion although this study employed a small sample size, it has effectively highlighted some of the potential dangers associated with non-formal and non-systematic use of informal translators in the translation of formal speech-language assessment tools. one may argue that the wab is not the most frequently used assessment tool, yet the authors believe that similar results would have been obtained had another formal test been translated, given that the nature of the differences in translation were mostly related to vocabulary and semantics. the findings suggested that test items may be affected by the language, culture, gender and level of education of the translator. this concurs with the findings of mosdell et al. (2010); however, at this point these factors cannot be considered as definitive because of the preliminary nature of the study. these factors therefore warrant more in-depth attention in future studies where bigger sample sizes can be used. while the effects that translation may have on the process of assessment of language impairments are undeniable, the solution to this is less clear. as slts we can continue to motivate for the employment of formal trained translators/interpreters, yet issues associated with the diversity of cultures and languages in the south african context may not be notably reduced in the presence of a translator. while the goal of employing a translator in cross-cultural consultations would be to reduce the effects of language barriers on the accurate assessment of speech and language impairments, this study produces evidence that mediated consultations may in fact be more complex in speech-language therapy than in general health communication, and warrant a different type of solution. efforts to develop and standardise assessment tools in african languages, coupled with increasing the numbers of slts who speak african languages, need to be intensified as it appears these would be the most reliable and logical ways of ensuring that appropriate and effective slt service delivery is achieved within this context. acknowledgement. the authors would like to thank dr i j mhlambi from the department of african languages, university of the witwatersrand, for her active participation in the current study.   references anderson, n. b. (1992). understanding cultural diversity. american journal of speech-language pathology, 1, 11-12. berndt, r. s. (2001). patterns of sentence comprehension in aphasia: a consideration of three hypotheses. journal of brain and language, 60(2), 197-221. bornman, j, sevcik, r. a., romski, m., & pae, h. k. (2010). successfully translating language and culture when adapting assessment measures. journal of policy and practice in intellectual disabilities, 7, 111-118. doi:10.1111/j.1741-1130.2010.00254.x davidson, b. (2001). questions in cross-linguistic medical encounters: the role of the hospital interpreter. anthropological quarterly, 74(4), 170-178. doi:10.1353/anq.2001.0035 department of health (2011). policy on language services (2011) for the national department of health. retrieved from http://www.doh.gov.za/docs/policy/2011/15042011.pdf. elderkin-thompson, v., silver, r. c., & waitzkin, h. (2001).when nurses double as interpreters: a study of spanish-speaking patients in a us primary care setting. social science and medicine, 52(9), 1343-1358. doi:10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00234-3 ercikan, k. (1998). translation effects in international assessments. international journal of educational research, 29(4), 543-553. doi:10.1016/s0883-0355(98)00047-0 flores, g., laws, m. b., mayo, s. j., zuckerman, b., abreu, m., medina, l., & hardt e. j. (2003). errors in medical interpretation and their potential clinical consequences in paediatric encounters. paediatrics, 111(1), 6-15. doi:10.1542/peds.111.1.6 foley, e. e. (2005). hiv/aids and african immigrant women in philadelphia: structural and cultural barriers to care. aids care, 17(8), 1030-1043. doi:10.1080/09540120500100890 friedland, d., & penn, c. (2003). conversation analysis as a technique for exploring the dynamics of a mediated interview. international journal of communication disorders, 38(1), 95-111. doi:10.1080/13682820304811 geisinger, k. f. (1994). cross-cultural normative assessment: translation and adaptation issues influencing the normative interpretation of assessment instruments. psychological assessment, 6(4), 304-312. doi:10.1037//1040-3590.6.4.304 gerber, b. (2009). a reflection on the development of a service-learning pedagogy for teaching and learning in speech-language therapy in south africa. in m. moore & p. lan lin (eds.), service-learning in higher education: paradigms and challenges (pp. 157-170). indianapolis: university of indianapolis press. kennedy, m. r. t., & yorkston, k. m. (2000). accuracy of metamemory after traumatic brain injury: predictions during verbal learning. journal of speech, language and hearing research, 43(5), 1072-1087. kertesz, a. (1982). the western aphasia battery. new york: grune & stratton. khoza k., ramma l., mophosho m., & moroka d. (2008). digit speech reception threshold testing in tswana/english speakers. south african journal of communication disorders, 55, 20-28. kuun, c., zimu, v., barnard, e., & davel, m. (2005). statistical investigations into isizulu intonation. human language technologies research, 1, 1-5. mochan, a., modi, m., & modi, g. (2003). stroke in black south african hiv-positive patients: a prospective analysis. stroke journal of american heart association, 34, 10-15. doi: 10.1161/01.str.0000043821.35051.fa mosdell, j., balchin, r., & ameen, o. (2010). adaptation of aphasia tests for neurocognitive screening in south africa. south african journal of psychology, 40(3), 250-261. penn, c. (2007). factors affecting the success of mediated medical interviews in south africa. current allergy & clinical immunology, 20(2), 66-72. ovretveit, j. (1998). making true comparisons. in j. ovretveit (ed.), comparative and cross-cultural health research: a practical guide. oxford: radcliffe medical press. doi:10.1093/eurpub/8.1.87 south african constitution. (1996). south african bill of rights. retrieved from http://www.rebirth.co.za/bill of rights.sa1.htm 3 february 2011. south african medical research council. (2003). general principles including research in children, vulnerable groups, international collaboration and epidemiology. retrieved from http://www.sahealthinfo.org/ethics/ethicspolicy.htm 28 january 2011 southern african stroke prevalence initiative (saspi) project team. (2004). prevalence of stroke survivors in rural south africa: results from the southern africa stroke prevalence initiative. stroke journal of the american heart association, 35, 627-632. spiggle, s. (1994). analysis and interpretation of qualitative data in consumer research. the journal of consumer research, 21(3): 491-503. doi:10.1086/209413 statistics south africa (2003). prevalence of disability in south africa. retrieved from http://www.statssa.gov.za 23 february 2009. statistics south africa (2010). mid-year population estimates, 2010. retrieved from http://www.statssa.gov.za/publications/p0302/p03022010.pdf 21 january 2011. stolk, y., ziguras, s., saunders, t., garlick, r., stuart, g., & coffey, g. (1998). lowering the language barrier in an acute psychiatric setting. australian and new zealand journal of psychiatry, 32(3), 434-440. doi:10.3109/00048679809065538 swedley, b. d., stith, a. y. & nelson, a. r. (2003). unequal treatment: confronting racial and ethnic disparities. washington, dc: national academies press. doi:10.1016/s0003-4975(03)01205-0 van schoor, o., van niekerk, j., & grobbelaar, b. (2001). mechanical failures as a contributing cause of motor vehicle accidents: south africa. accident analysis and prevention, 33 (6), 713-721. doi:10.1016/s0001-4575(00)00083-x 6 9 kurrikulumontwikkeling vir spraak-taalterapi oudiologie: basis en beginsels s.r. hugo, d. phil (pretoria) i.c. uys, d. phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming tuur nie langer voldoen aan die multikulturele, 'h ^lor^sonta^e beroepstruk natiewe opleidmgsmoontlikhede soos diploma-oplddingv^mn^^serti^cmtn^n nodig omte besin oor die instelling van alter gerigte magisteropleiding. in samehang hierje het berlp.z^ en beroepsgemeenskapsdiens en konsultasie η veel groter rol in ΐίιζτζζι behoon ^ ^ — abstract ekenaargeletterdheid, bestuursfunksies te speel as wat tans die geval is. ^ ΐ ϊ β ί o f c u r u l u m d e v d o p m e n t f o r « the multicultural, multilingual rsa context. it is therefore necessaru to inv^tim^ah f0''01^^0'1'1^structure ^ n o l°n0er adequate in needs. these may include diploma trainingfor techniaans certifirafe nrn alternative educational options to accommodate current directed masters courses. it is also apparentthattk^ofessionctifan^ wor^lers and professionally the point where aspects such as computer literacy, mmi^em^fiukti^^mmmimh αηί^ audiologist have extended to curriculum thin is the case at present. ^ementfunawns, community work and consultation should play a greater role in the inleiding >' ! zlxzi^' l f a t l v e i n ™akms thin«s ^ tolim^t t 0 m h e c a u s e o f o u r o w n b e n m n e 3 l e a hierdie stalling is verteenwo'ordigend van die algemene gees wat tans m d l e beroep van spraakheelkunde en oudiolog e kottek v s e g t e r , ° ° k t 6 k e n e n d v a " t w e e belangrike kontekse, te wete die universiteitswese in die a l g e m e e n en die suider-afrikaanse samelewing in die besonder teen hierd raamwerk is dit seker nie vreemd dat die behoefte van kurriku lumontwikkehng vir die opleiding van spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe so sterk na vore gekom het nie. ? die fundamentele vraag wat beantwoord moet word is· wat is die spesifieke beroepsen opleidingsveranderinge wattn 'n ' opleidingskurrikulum vervat moet word? kurr1kuleringsveranderinge· internasionale gesigspunte ' kridesep11^ γ ^ 1 9 8 ° " 1 9 9 ° d e k a d e w o r d ^ e n m e r k deur kritiese evaluas,e van sowel die beroep as die bestaande opleida"aimrds w a t c ' e u r i t i ' d d e ' v 3 n o m n ' b u s o p n a m e s data o o r die stand van spraakterapie en oudiologie a s beroep ingesamel het. die opleiding is dan geevalueer in terme varuhe die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 37, 1990 beroepseise. sodoende is verskeie voorgraadse en nagraadse kursusse, asook voortgesette onderrig geweeg in terme van relevansie van 'n ontwikkelende en veranderende beroep hyman, 1986; shewan, 1988; terrizzi, 1988). uit die britse literatuur blyk dit ook dat oplossings vir diensleweringspros e v i n d k a n w o r d deur die behoefte aan diens en die voorsiening van diens vas te stel (enderby & davies, 1989). relevante demografiese gegewens in verband met die stand van die beroep dui op klemverskuiwings wat leemtes met betrekking tot beroepsen bestuursfiinksies laat. dit is veral die internasionale menseregteverdrae en die standpunte van die gesondheidsorganisasie wat nuwe insigte gebring en orientasieveranderinge laat plaasvind het. opsommenderwys kan die volgende riglyne vir verandering aangestip word: 5 vir ongeveer twee dekades word die behoefte aan spraakterapie en oudiologie as 'n enkel-professie bepleit (feldman 1981). alhoewel daar geleidelike verandering in die opleidingsprogramme te bespeur is, is die algemene tendens vir die vsa steeds om eerder as oudioloog of spraakterapeut te kwalifiseer, te registreer en te werk. demografiese bevindings dui egter aan dat die gevallebelading in die praktyk deskundigheid op beide gebiede vereis en noop die aanvaarding van stellings soos: «true specialization grows 0 savsg 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 s.r. hugo & i.c. uys out of generalist training" (feldman, 1981: 942). * daar is vir baie jare reeds 'n behoefte om 'n beroepskorps daar te stel wat spraakterapie en oudiologie tot 'n onafhanklike beroep kan lei. alhoewel die oorgrote meerderheid spraakterapeute en oudioloe steeds by instansies soos skole en hospitale diens lewer, is daar tans 'n omskakeling na privaat praktyk te bespeur (shewan, 1988). hierdie oorskuiwing word deur asha ondersteun maar hulle beklemtoon ook die feit dat opleiding hierby aangepas moet word. * verwant aan die bogemelde punt, is 'n behoefte dat spraakterapeute en oudioloe onmiddellik na opleiding (met toetrede tot die beroepslewe) oor al die nodige beroepsen bestuursvaardighede sal beskik om onafhanklik en toerekenbaar in die privaat praktyk op te tree. opleidingsprogramme moet dus voorsiening maak dat studente daardie kritiese korpus van kennis en vaardighede bekom voor toetrede tot die beroep (feldman, 1981; lingwall, 1988). * juis as gevolg van die veranderende werksomstandighede het dit nodig geword om spraakterapeute en oudioloe vakinhoudelik meer omvangryk voor te berei op hulle beroepsfunksies (flower, 1984). in die basiese opleiding moet aandag geskenk word aan die kwantiteit en kwaliteit van die teoretiese en kliniese opleiding, die verhouding tussen fundamentele geesteswetenskaplike en natuurwetenskaplike inhoude; die verhouding tussen basiese en toegepaste wetenskappe; die verhouding tussen akademiesteoretiese en kliniese opleiding (terrizzi, 1988). * feldman (1981) identifiseer die behoefte aan voortgesette onderrig en veral kliniese magistergrade en doktorsgrade. opleidingsinstansies moet ook voortgesette onderrig aan oudstudente, wat nie noodwendig graad-kwalifikasies is nie, as 'n verpligting beskou omdat die vakgebied so vinnig ontwikkel (hyman, 1986; shewan, 1988). * een van die grootste behoeftes wat deur verskeie ondersoeke gei'dentifiseer is, is die onkunde oor bestuursfunksies wat tans by spraakterapeute en oudioloe bestaan (foxman, 1988; gelatt, 1988; griffin, 1988; holley, 1988). die aanbeveling word gemaak dat veral die bemarking van dienste aandag moet geniet, omdat dit al hoe meer deel word van die eise wat aan lewensvatbare beroepe gestel word. * verskeie behoeftes het ontstaan as gevolg van politieke druk in samehang met tegnologiese ontwikkeling. die belangrikste hiervan is: die behoefte aan rekenaargeletterdheid. die rekenaar word al hoe meer onontbeerlik in die beoefening van spraakterapie en oudiologie. gedurende 1984 het ongeveer 27% spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe rekenaars gebruik; en 'n opname in 1988 dui op 'n styging na amper 58%. dit is interessant dat die reke. n a a r i n 1986 hoofsaaklik vir bestuursfunksies (administrasie, rekordhouding en woordverwerking) aangewend is, terwyl die 1988-opname aantoon dat rekenaars tans hoofsaaklik vir diagnostiese en behandelingsdoeleindes gebruik word. opleiding in rekenaargebruik word derhalwe al hoe meer 'n noodsaaklikheid (hyman, 1986; shewan, 1988). die betrokkenheid by minderheidsgroepe. reeds in 1985 het asha die standpunt ingeneem dat hulp verleen behoort te word aan persone in uiteenlopende sosiale, kulturele, ekonomiese en linguistiese populasies. alhoewel opleiding van terapeute uit minderheidsgroepe vanaf 1987 spesiale aandag geniet, is daar huidiglik in die vs a slegs 3% geregistreerde spraaktaalterapeute en oudioloe uit hierdie bevolkingsgroepe. opnames toon egter dat terapeute al hoe meer betrokke raak by die hantering van minderheidsgroepe (cole en massey, 1985; hyman, 1986; shewan, 1988). die betrokkenheid by spesiale groepe as gevolg van die ontwikkeling van die wetenskap. as gevolg van die identifikasie van kommunikasieafwykings by spesifieke groepe soos taalleergestremdes, vigslyers, verskeie neuropatologiee en selfs bejaardes (met die toename in hierdie populasie), brei die spraakterapeut en oudioloog se werksomvang skielik uit. opleidingsinstansies moet derhalwe voorsiening maak vir die voortgesette onderrig van persone wat in die verlede gekwalifiseer het ten einde hulle vaardighede na die nuwe toepassingsvelde uit te brei. kurrikuleringsveranderinge: nasionale vertrekpunte dit is opmerklik dat die plaaslike literatuur 'n byna identiese weerspieeling van oorsese veranderinge aandui. dit is egter te verstane omdat die vsa-model vir baie dekades al aangepas en toegepas is in die rsa. juis hierdie basis behoort in 'n evaluasie van bestaande opleiding bevraagteken te word. soos delaney en malan (1984:75) dit stel: "this foreign model has been modified to provide our south african model. this is not to sag that a foreign model modified on the basis of limited information is necessarily inadequate, but rather that it is important that we be aware that this is the situation." 'n eiesoortige probleemsituasie dra by tot die noodsaaklikheid van 'n rsa-opleidingsmodel. ten eerste is dit die kenmerke van die ontwikkelende land wat eiesoortige aanpassings vir dienslewering en opleiding noodsaak. penn (1986) bevraagteken die relevansie van opleiding in hierdie omstandighede, i omdat daar tans 'n "ongesonde status en beeld van die beroep 1 as oorwegend 'n luukse bestaan" (uys, 1987). in samehang ' hiermee bepleit aron (1984(a) en 198 7) veranderinge in opleiding en dienslewering om aan hierdie behoeftes van, onder andere, minderbevoorregting, armoede, onkunde en selfs ongeletterdheid te voldoen, deur alternatiewe opleiding binne die raamwerk van gemeenskapsgebaseerde benaderings aan te bied. sy staan ook die opleiding van primere sorgpersone voor. j fourie (1984) ondersteun hierdie siening dat aanpassing by j die eise van 'n derde wereldland die opleiding en aanstelling van semigekwalifiseerde persone onder toesig noodsaak. daarenteen meen morgan, geraghty, dawber, motshei, drew, motshei en segal (1984) dat 'n gemeenskapsorientasie nie aan 'n afs. nderlike kursus, of 'n afsonderlike groep werkers met anc er funksies toegese behoort te word nies maar dat 'n gemeenskapsorientasie geinkorporeer behoort te word in die opleiding van spraakterapeute en oudioloe. die implikasies van hierdie voorstelle is eerstens dat die tradisionele benadering tot opleiding (die identifikasie van patologie in die individu en die rehabilitasie van die idividu) en dienslewering (die uitvoering van die praktyk binne die raamwerk van institusionele strukture, bv. hospitale, skole, ens.) ontoepaslik is. voorbereiding van die spraakterapeut en y the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) kurrikulumontwikkeling vir spraak-taalterapie en oudiologie: basis en begin se is 71 oudioloog om by toetrede tot die praktyk onafhanklik toerekenbare diens te lewer, plaas 'n hoe premie op opleiding veral met betrekking tot omvattende primere, sekondere en tersiere voorkoming en konsultasie (uys, 1985). die tweede implikasie is dat spraakterapeute en oudioloe 'n navorsingsinset sal moet lewer en dus 'n sterker akademiese opleiding in wetenskapmetodologie sal moet ontvang om hulle voor te berei op sinvolle navorsing (uys, 1986; 1987). 'n kompliserende faktor wat die eise van 'n onwikkelende land verder verhoog is die geografiese kenmerke van die rsa. die uitgestrektheid van plattelandse gebiede en die groot bevolkingsamevloeiing ver van sentrale gebiede is vroeg al as 'n besondere kenmerk van dienslewering gei'dentifiseer. reeds in 1966 onderstreep pienaar die behoefte aan "properly conducted surveys at regular intervals over large areas" en "clinics, mobile and otherwise established in ... country districts" (in morgan et al. 1984: 111). in opleiding stel dit die eis dat klem gele moet word op gemeenskapsdiens, konsultasie, en veel eerder generalisasie as spesialisasie. noodwendig hang geografiese kenmerke nou saam met die beskikbaarheid van dienste. in 1984 was daar na beraming reeds 'n tekort aan 4,494 spraakterapeute en oudioloe in die rsa (fourie, 1984). in 1986 het die tekort gestyg tot oor die 5 000 en volgens projeksie (met inagneming van die bevolkingsgroei) sal daar in 2000 'n tekort aan minstens 10 000 wees (gestremdheid in die republiek van suid-afrika, 1987(a), (b), (c)). dit het verder aan die lig gekom dat bykans alle dienste uitsluitlik gerig is op die identifikasie, diagnose en behandeling van bevoorregte, stedelike blankes, dat dienste aan die ontwikkelende plattelandse bevolkingsgroepe totaal ontoereikend is en dat voorkomingsdienste feitlik nerens beskikbaar is nie (gestremdheid in die republiek van suid-afrika, 1987(c); aron, 1984(a)). verskeie aanbevelings is reeds gemaak om hierdie probleem te oorkom, naamlik: i die verhoging van studentetalle uit alle bevolkingsgroepe /by bestaande opleidingsinstansies (uys, 1984). die instelling van opleidingskursusse by ander (addisionele) universiteite, met veral die oog op opleiding van persone uit asier-, kleurlingen swart-bevolkingsgroepe. daar is tans ongeveer 4 kleurling-, 25 asieren 4 swartspraakterapeute en oudioloe geregistreer (gestremdheid in die republiek van suid-afrlika,-1987(c)). met inagneming van die bevolkingstoename van hierdie drie groepe, is die opleiding van spraakterapeute en oudioloe uit hierdie groepe noodsaaklik (penn, 1986; crossley, 1987). die aanpassing van opleidingskursusse met die oog daarop om terapeute in staat te stel om groter pasientgetalle te hanteer deur alternatiewe spraakterapeutiese en oudiologiese benadering (morgan et al. 1984; hugo & louw, 1987), konsultasie (uys, 1985) en die gebruik van ondersteuningsdienste (enderby & davies, 1989). die multikulturele, veeltalige bevolkings van die rsa skep 'n behoefte in terme van die omvang en kwaliteit van die bestaande dienste. die opleiding en kwalifikasies van spraakterapteute en oudioloe kan, met spesiale verwysing na hulle rol in 'n meertalige, multikulwele gemeenskap, gekritiseer word. die opleiding voldoen huidiglik beslis nie aan al die eise wat deur hierdie samelewing gestel word nie (uys, 1984). die habilitasie en rehabilitasie van kommunikasieafwykings is anders as ander terapiee in die opsig dat kommunikasie beide die middel en die doel in behandeling is. die spraakterapeuten oudioloog moet nie slegs oor 'n diepgaande kennis van die taal van die pasient beskik nie, maar ook 'n goeie begrip van die sosiokulturele eienskappe van die betrokke bevolkingsgroep he (aron, 1984 (b); crossley, 1984). verskeie voorstelle word voorgehou ter oplossing van hierdie probleem: om basiese konsepte van swarten indiertale in te sluit in die opleiding van spraakterapeute en oudioloe. om inligting in verband met sosiokulturele verskille in te sluit in die opleiding van spraakterapeute en oudioloe (crossley, 1984; segal & drew, 1984). om in spanverband navorsing te doen oor hierdie verskille en verskynsels, sodat norme vir kommunikasiegedrag op alle vlakke vasgestel, en diagnostiese en behandelingsmateriaal ontwikkel kan word (aron, 1987; webb, 1984). om van spraakterapieen oudiologie-assistente, of tolke gebruik te maak in dienslewering (uys, 1984). om spraakterapeute en oudioloe vanuit hierdie groepe op te lei (penn, 1986). hierdie behoefte aan verandering, om opleiding en dienslewering aan te pas by die rsa-konteks, is ook reeds deur die outeurs gei'dentifiseer in 'n navorsingsprojek. dit blyk uit vraelyste wat aan 'n verteenwoordigende populasie van spraak-taal-gehoorterapeute gestuur is, asook uit onderhoude wat met die persone gevoer is, dat 73% aanpassing van die opleiding by rsa-omstandighede voorstaan, terwyl 62% voorstanders is van verandering in opleiding om voorsiening te maak vir klemverskuiwing met betrekking tot die beroepsfunksies (hugo & uys, 1988). die voorafgaande inligting skets op 'n gekondenseerde wyse enkele van die vraagstukke rondom opleiding en meer spesifiek die probleem van kurrikulering vir spraakterapie en oudiologie. indien dit die probleem is wat is die oplossings? tentatiewe oplossings vir kurrikulums kurrikulumontwikkeling is die "doelgerigte en sistematiese opbou van die kurrikulum en die voortdurende evaluering, herevaluering en vernuwing daarvan" (kachelhoffer, 1987: 14). indien die inligting in die voorafgaande paragrawe as vertrekpunt gebruik word, is dit duidelik dat die bestaande opleiding van spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe in die rsa redelik indringend verander moet word. hierdie veranderinge sal natuurlik ook in samehang met die eiesoortige karakter van die inrigting waar die opleiding aangebied word, moet ontwikkel. daar is egter twee belangrike fundamentele aspekte wat as basies tot enige kurrikulumontwikkeling gesien kan word. die beroepstruktuur en opleiding ten eerste is dit vir die internasionale aanvaarding van beroepsbeoefenaars belangrik dat die aard en kwaliteit van opleiding sodanig sal wees dat professionele erkenning maklik die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivgkings, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 7 2 s.r. hugo & i.c. uys plaasvind en uitruiling van studente 'n praktiese moontlikheid sal wees. derhalwe is 'n vierjarige beroepsgerigte baccalaureusgraad met hoofvakke spraak-taalpatologie en oudiologie 'n minimum vereiste vir registrasie as spraakterapeut en oudioloog. dit kan dan gevolg word met 'n magisteren doktorsgraad vir bykomende kwalifikasie en spesialisasie. 'n ander vorm van professionele graadopleiding vir registrasiedoeleindes moet egter oorweeg word. dit is die moontlikheid van 'n tweeof driejarige, beroepsgerigte magistergraad in spraak-taalpatologie of oudiologie, met as voorvereiste enige algemene baccalaureusgraad. hierdie opleiding sou 'n verintensifisering van die basiese opleiding beteken met gevolglike registrasie as of spraakterapeut of oudioloog. die voordeel van sodanige opleiding is dat dit volwasse en gemotiveerde studente sal lok wat waarskynlik uit 'n spesifieke beroepsopset (bv. skole vir gehoorgestremdes of serebraalgestremdes), 'n spesifieke belangstelling in die kommunikasiegestremde ontwikkel het. kurrikulering en beroepsfiinksies wanneer die jaarboeke van opleidingsinstansies bestudeer word, is dit duidelik dat opleiding by universiteite tans grootliks 'n afwykinggerigte (bv. hakkel, breinbesering, ens.) benadering volg. dit beteken dat hoofsaaklik twee van die beroepsfunksies, diagnose en terapie, uitvoerig onderrig word, dikwels ten koste van die meeste ander beroepsfunksies. die resultaat hiervan is 'n onewewigtige kurrikulum wat nie met die huidige beroepseise tred hou nie. ten einde 'n kurrikulum te ontwikkel wat 'n funksionele holistiese basis het, is dit nodig om alle beroepsfunksies te lys en te definieer. hierdie funksies word in tabel 1 uiteengesit. tabel 1: beroepsfunksies van spraak-taalterapeute oudioloe ten tweede is dit so dat omstandighede in die rsa dui op die ondervoorsiening van dienste in die algemeen, maar spesifiek ook met betrekking tot diens aan ander kultuurgroepe, ander taalgroepe, die geriatriese en die plattelandse bevolking. dit is derhalwe duidelik dat die verhoging van basiese graadopleiding alleen nie die antwoord op hierdie probleem sal wees nie. die oplossing hiervoor is die instelling van ander vorms van diens wat dienooreenkomstig ander tipes van opleiding benodig. dit word visueel voorgestel in figuur 1, en bestaan in essensie uit die volgende: graadopleiding aangebied deur universiteite vir die kwalifikasie van terapeute wat kan optree as probleemoplossers, toesighouers, konsultante en spesialiste. diploma-opleiding aangebied deur technikons vir die kwalifikasie van terapeutiese assistente en spraaken gehoorgemeenskapswerkers wat gemoeid sal wees met die aanbieding van roetine/tegniese aspekte van kommunikasiepatologie. sertifikaatprogramme aangebied deur diensleweringsorganisasies vir die kwalifikasie van gemeenskapsrehabilitasiewerkers. sentrale klinieke . spesialis spraakterapeute en oudioloe ' (nagraadse spesialisasie)\ opleiding: universiteit gedesentraliseerde klinieke spraakterapeute en/of oudioloe (baccalaureusgraad, en ma in spraak-taalpatologie of oudiologie) opleiding: universiteit gemeenskappe> semi-gekwalifiseerdes spraaken gehoorgemeenskapswerkers (gediplomeerdes) opleiding: technikon gemeenskapsrehabilitasiewerkers (sertifikaatprogramme) opleiding: diensleweringsorganisasies beroepsfunksie omskrywing 1 sifting aktiwiteite wat uitgevoer word ten einde uit 'n grootpopulasie te differensieer tussen persone wat normale kommunikasie het en diegene wat vollediger bestudeer moet word om vas te stel of hulle 'n spesifieke afwyking het. 2 voorkoming aktiwiteite wat uitgevoer word ten einde die aanvang, ontwikkeling en komplikasie van 'n kommunikasieafwyking te elimineer en/of te inhibeer. 3 gemeenskapswerk aktiwiteite wat daarop gerig is om 'n diens aan en deur die gemeenskap te lewer ten einde kommunikasieafwykings te voorkom, te diagnoseer en te behandel. 4 terapie 5 evaluasie figuur 1: opleidingsmodel vir die beroepstruktuur aktiwiteite wat poog om kommunikasiegedrag te modifieer. hier kan drie tipes aktiwiteite onderskei word: gerig op verwerking van elemente van normale kommunikasie * gerig op verwerwing van kompensatoriese prosedures in gevalle van onherstelbare afwykings * gerig op verandering van weerstand teen kommunikasieverbetering. aktiwiteite wat ten doel het om 'n kommunikasie-afwyking te beskiyf of om beslissings aangaande hulpverlen ing te maak. die doel hiervan is: * om aanwesigheid en graad van afwyking te bepaal * om die afwyking te beskryf * om die stappe ter oplossing aan te dui * om verwysingsraamwerk vir intervensiegevolge te stel(prognose) * om aan ander professies die implikasies van die afwyking aan te toon. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) kurrikulumontwikkeling vir spraak-taalterapie en oudiologie: basis en beginsels tabel 1 vervolg 73 6 beraad 7 bestuur 8 konsultasie 9 navorsing 10 onderrig dienste gebied aan die kommunikasieafwykende en/of sy familie (en belangrike omgewingsindividue) gerig op die oplossing of vermindering van probleme wat met die kommunikasieafwyking verband hou of daaruit spruit. hierdie dienste vul terapie aan ten opsigte van: * beraad aan die kommunikasieafwykende om ander vorms van professionele hulp te soek en te aanvaar * beraad aan familielede ten einde terapie te steun en hulle kommunikatiewe rol aan te dui. aktiwiteite wat uitgevoer word om menslike finansiele, fisiese en inligtingshulpbronne te benut ten einde die onderneming (ondernemings wat met die kommunikasieafwykende gemoeid is) se doel witte aan te wend. dit sluit in bestuurfunksies soos beplanning, organisasie, koordinering en beheer. aktiwiteite wat daarop gerig is om aan ander professionele persone 'n diens te lewer ten bate van die kommunikasieafwykende. aktiwiteite wat daarop gerig is om tot grondige en akkurate kennis en insig van normale en afwykende menslike kommunikasie te kom. die opleiding en opvoeding van persone in normale en afwykende menslike kommunikasie in terme van kennis, vaardighede en gesindhede wat daarme.e verband hou. in^fabel 1 is gepoog om alle beroepsfunksies, ongeag die spesifieke arbeidsterrein, en toepaslik op so wel spraakterapeute as oudioloe, uit te spel. uiteindelik sal dit noodwendig van die spesifieke werkgewer en werksomstandighede afhang watter funksie/s die meeste beklemtojon word, maar vir kurrikulumontwikkeling is dit nodig om alle fasette in die sillabus te be trek. ! i beroepstruktuur en beroepsfunksies aangesien die voorgestelde beroepstruktuur, sowel as die gedifferensieerde opleiding van verskeie faktore afhanklik is, kan daar tans slegs pro-aktiewe voorstelle gemaak word oor die versoening tussen opleiding, struktuur en funksies. die funksies (bv. sifting, behandeling, voorkoming ens.) benodig. die eerste kwalifikasie in spraak-taalterapie en oudiologie sal daarenteen weer 'n algemeen gemiddelde kennis en vaardigheid met betrekking tot alle funksies as voorvereiste stel. namate'styging in die hierargie plaasvind, kan 'n stygende tendens in indiepte-kennis met betrekking tot bestuur, konsultasie, onderrig ens. 'n voorvereiste wees, terwyl spesialisasie deur navorsing, 'n getande profiel aan opleidingsomvang kan verskaf. kurrikulum quo vadis? alles is al vantevore bedink. die noodsaak is weer daaraan te dink. dit is 'n algemeen geldige stelling. die voorafgaande bespreking lig enkele belangrike tendense uit waarvoor toegelaat moet word in kurrikulumontwikkeling. kurrikulumontwikkeling is egter nie 'n horisontale groei nie maar vind veelvlakkig plaas. op die makrovlak sal daadwerklik gekyk moet word na die verskillende vlakke van opleiding (insluitend technikons en diensleweringsinstansies) sodat mindere en andersoortige kwalifikasies as voorvereiste vir omvattender en meer koste-effektiewe dienslewering daargestel kan word. mesovlakverandering betrek die spesifieke departemente en sluit in kursuskurrikulering. hier is dit essensieel dat die onderskeie departemente kursusse ontwikkel wat eerstens aan die universele behoeftes van die beroep voldoen, maar tweedens toelaat vir die spesifieke karakter van die bepaalde universiteit en die geografiese gemeenskap wat die universiteit bedien. daar moet veral ook aandag gegee word aan die rol wat nagraadse onderrig, ondersteuningsprogramme en voortgesette onderrig binne die bree kursuskurrikulum speel. wat die mikrovlak betref, moet volledig besin word oor sillabustemas, en lesingeenhede, maar sinvolle beplanning hier sal van waarde wees indien dit in samehang met onderrigdidaktiek en personeelorganisasie gedoen word. die uiteindelike sukses van kurrikulumontwikkeling is in die laaste instansie in die hande van diegene wat die onderrig moet aanbied. dit beteken dat hoe eise aan dosente en ander opleiers gestel sal word. van hulle sal kwaliteite vereis word soos aanpasbaarheid, deeglike vakkennis, hoe intelligensie, entoesiasme, bereidwilligheid om te leer en te groei, en selfs enkele nie-vakverwante vaardighede en gesindhede. hierdie vereiste eienskappe is in elk geval kenmerkend van toegewyde en professionele spraakterapeute en oudioloe. daar is derhalwe geen twyfel nie dat die beroepslui met gemak sal kan voldoen aan die uitdagings wat kurrikulumontwikkeling stel. alhoewel dit met die eerste oogopslag mag voorkom asof alle kurrikulums slegs swaarder gelaai word met vakinhoud, is dit glad nie die geval nie. grondliggend aan die opleiding is andersoortige opleidingsorientasies en opleidingsmetodieke. klemverskuiwing met betrekking tot die gewig van insette oor die funksies sal die verskillende struktuurvlakke onderskei. dit word aanvaar dat al die funksies in 'n meerdere of mindere mate op elke beroepsvlak ter sprake moet kom. basiese riglyne kan egter voorgestel word. basisvlakwerkers sal waarskynlik beperkte kennis en vaardigheid met betrekking tot sekere van verwysings aron, m.l. introduction. in m.l. aron en l. ie roux (reds) proceedings of the conference on community work in speech and hearing therapy. savsg, 1-5, 1984(a). aron, m.l. community work in speech therapy. in s.m. beukes (red.) simposiumreferate: die rol van die spraakterapeut in 'n meertalige samelewing. universiteit van pretoria, 10-16, 1984(b). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafins, vol. 37 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 7 4 s.r. hugo & i.c. uys aron, m.l. community-based rehabilitation for communication disorders. in w. smit en s. meyer (reds.) nasionale konferensieverrigtinge. savsg (ongenommer), 1987. cole, p.r.j want to shape my own future. how about you? asha, 28(a), 41-42, 1986. cole, l. en massey, a. minority student enrolment in higher education institutions with communicative disorders programs. asha, 27(6), 33-37, 1985. crossley, s. language remediation in an indian community. in s.m. beukes (red.) simposiumreferate: die rol van die spraakterapeut in 'n meertalige samelewing. universiteit van pretoria, 38-61, 1984. crossley, s. how to train clinicians to work with culturally different clients. communiphon, 280, 2-11, nov./des. 1987. delaney, c. en malan, k. community speech and hearing therapy: some questions before answers. in m.l. aron en l. ie roux (reds.) proceedings of the conference on communitg work in speech and hearing therapg. savsg, 73-84, 1984. enderby, p. en davies, p. communication disorders: planning a service to meet the needs. british journal of disorders of communication, 24, 301-331, 1989. feldman, a.s. the challenge of autonomy. asha, 23(12), 941945, 1981. flower, r.m. deliverg of speech-language pathology and audiology services. baltimore: williams en wilkens, 1984. fourie, h.p. maatskaplike behoeftebepaling. in s.m. beukes (red.) simpodum-refer ate: die rol van die spraakterapeut in 'n meertalige samelewing. universiteit van pretoria, 2-9, 1984. foxman, c.a. speak with sense. asha, 30(9), 46-47, 1988. gelatt, p.j. the business of grantseeking. asha, 30(9), 43-45, 1988. gestremdheid in die republiek van suid-afrika: hoofverslag 1, pretoria: departement van gesondheid en bevolkingsontwikkeling, 1987(a). gestremdheid in die republiek van suid-afrika: gehoorgestremdheid 8, pretoria: departement van gesondheid en bevolkingsontwikkeling, 1987(b). gestremdheid in die republiek van suid-afrika: spraakgestremdheid 10, pretoria: departement van gesondheid en bevolkingsontwikkeling, 1987(c). griffin, k.m. quality sells, asha, 30(9), 48-51, 1988. holley, s.c. president's page asha, 30(9), 37-38, 1988. hugo, s.r. en louw, b. ouergesentreerde interaksieterapie. in w. smit en s. meyer (reds.) nasionale konferensie-verrigtinge, savsg, (ongenommer), 1987. hugo, s.r. en uys, i.c. kurrikulering van spraakheelkunde en oudiologie. in referategelewer tgdens die oudiologiekontires. savsg, 190-201, 1988. hyman, c.s. the 1985-omnibus survey. implications for strategic planning. asha, 28(4), 19-22, 1986. kachelhoffer, p.m. kurrikulumontwikkeling: riglyne aan kurrikulumkomitees. up-dosent, (1), 14-20, 1987. lingwall, j.b. report: evaluation of the requirements for the certificates of clinical competence in speech-language pathology and audiology. asha, 30(9), 75-78, 1988. morgan, r., geraghty, s., dawber, α., motshei, f., drew, m., motshei, m. en segal, d. an alternative approach to speech therapy. in m.l. aron en l. ie roux (reds.) proceedings of the conference on community work in speech and hearing therapg. savsg, 1 ιοι 20, 1984. penn; c. guest editorial. communiphon, 274:2, 1986. segal, df. en drew, m. argument for a community oriented approach to language therapy. in s.m. beukes (red.) simposiumreferate: die rol van die spraakterapeut in 'n meertalige samelewing. universiteit van pretoria, 88-92, 1984. shewan, c.m. 1988-omnibussurvey: adaptation and progress in times of change. asha, 30(8), 27-32, 1988. terrizzi, a.m. status report on undergraduate education in communication sciences and disorders. asha, 30(5), 31-33, 1988. uys, i.c. spraaken gehoorheelkunde in 'n meertalige samelewing. in s.m. beukes (red.) simposiumreferate: die rol van die spraakterapeut in 'n meertalige samewelinq: universiteit van pretoria, 88-93, 1984. uys, i.c. 'n medekonsultasiemodel vir spraaken gehoorterapie in die rsa. in i.s. hay en i.c. uys taalverskeidenheid en taalpatologie. pretoria: universiteit van pretoria, 1985. uys, i.c. gas redaksioneel. communiphon. 280, 1-2, 1986. uys, i.c. navorsing: 'n diens aan en deur die klinikus. in w. smit en s. meyer (reds.) nasionale konferensie-verrigtinge. savsg, (ongenommer), 1987. webb, v.n. sosiolinguistiek en die spraakterapie. in s.m. beukes (red.) simposiumreferate: die rol van die spraakterapeut in 'n meertalige samelewing. universiteit van pretoria, 23-27, 1984. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, or critically evaluative theoretical, or therapeutic issues dealing with disorders of speech, voice, hearing or language, or on aspects of the processes underlying these. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. all contributions are reviewed by at least two consultants who are not provided with author identification. form of manuscript. authors should submit four neatly typewritten manuscripts in triple spacing with wide margins which should not exceed much more than 25 pages. each page should be numbered. the first page of two copies should contain the title of the article, name of author/s, highest degree and address or institutional affiliation. the first page of the remaining two copies should contain only the title of the article. the second page of all copies should contain only an abstract (100 words) which should be provided in both english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors. all paragraphs should start at the left margin and not be indented. major headings, where applicable, should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements and references. tables and figures should be prepared on separate sheets (one per table/ figure). figures, graphs and line drawings must be originals, in blackink on good quality white paper. lettering appearing on these should be uniform and professionally done, bearing in mind that such lettering should be legible after a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should not be included in the illustration but should appear below it. the titles of tables and figures should be concise but explanatory. the title of tables appears above, and of figures below. tables and figures should be numbered in order of appearance (with arabic numerals). the amount of tabular and illustrative material allowed will be at the discretion of the editor (usually not more than 6). references. references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and date, e.g. van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, et al. after the first author will suffice. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list. references should be listed alphabetically in triple-spacing at the end of the article. for acceptable abbreviations of names of journals, consult the fourth issue (october) of dsh abstracts or the world list of scientific periodicals. the number of references used should not exceed much more than 20. note the following examples: locke, j.l. clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders./ speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology, 3rd ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stutteriny. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. proofs: galley proofs will be sent to the author wherever possible. corrections other than typographical errors will be charged to the author. reprints. 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. all manuscripts and correspondence should be addressed to: the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech and hearing association. p.o. box 31782, braamfontein 2017, south africa. inligting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykinys publiseer verslae en artikels oor navorsing, of krities evaluerende artikels oor die teoretiese of terapeutiese aspekte van spraak-, stem-, gehoorof taalafwykings, of oor aspekte van die prosesse onderliggend aan hierdie afwykings. > die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings sal nie materiaal aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. alle bydraes word deur minstens twee konsultante nagegaan wat nie ingelig is oor die identiteit van die skrywer nie. formaat van die manuskrip. skrywers moet vier netjies getikte manuskripte in 3-spasiering en met bree kantlyn indien, en dit moet nie veel langer as 25 bladsye wees nie. elke bladsy moet genommer wees. op die eerste bladsy van 2 afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, die naam van die skrywer/s, die hoogste graad behaal en die adres of naam van hulle betrokke instansie verskyn. op die eerste bladsy van die oorblywende twee afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskyn. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. alle paragrawe moet teenaan die linkerkantlyn begin word en moet nie ingekeep word nie. hoofopskrifte moet, waar dit van toepassing is, in die volgende volgorde wees: metode, resultate, bespreking, gevolg trekking, erkennings en verwysings. tabelle en figure moet op afsonderlike bladsye verskyn (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie). figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge moet oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte gedoen word. letterwerk wat hierop verskyn moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50%-verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by die illustrasie moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verklarings of omskrywings moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maar daaronder verskyn. die byskrifte van tabelle moet bo-aan verskyn en die van figure onderaan. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word (met arabiese syfers). die hoeveelheid materiaal in die vorm van tabelle en illustrasies wat toegelaat word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). venvysinys. verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, bv. van riper (1971). waar daar meer as twee skrywers is, sal et al. na die eerste skrywer voldoende wees. die name van alle skrywers moet in die verwysingslys verskyn. verwysings moet alfabeties in 3-spasiering aan die einde van die artikel gerangskik word. vir die aanvaarde afkortings van tydskrifte se titels, raadpleeg die vierde uitgawe (oktober) van dsh abstracts of the world list of scientific periodicals. die getal verwysings wat gebruik is, moet nie veel meer as 20 wees nie. let op die volgende voorbeelde: locke, j.l. clinical phonology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders./ speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. inj. katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audioloyy, 3e ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice hall, 1971. proewe: galeiproewe sal waar moontlik aan skrywers gestuur word. die onkoste van veranderings, behalwe tipografiese foute, sal deur die skrywer self gedra moet word. herdrukke. 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis verskaf word. alle manuskripte en korrespondensie moet gerig word aan: die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykinys. die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraaken gehoorheelkunde, posbus 31782, braamfontein 2017", suid-afrika. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) immediate echolalia and the interactive behaviour of autistic children ingrid van zyl b.(log) (pret) erna alant d.phil (pret) isabel c. uys d.phil (pret) department speech pathology and audiology, university of pretoria this research examined whether echolalia and interactive behaviour in autistic children could function within a communicative system. four autistic children were videotaped individually in interaction with a familiar adult. a categorical system was designed whereby the children s interactive behaviour could be rated. it was found that the immediate echolalia is far more than a meaningless repetition of words the utterances are relevant, displaying the conveyance of meaningful information and the maintenance of social interaction. conclusions were drawn on the facilitation of communication through modification and expansion of immediate echolalia. opsomming , , ., . . , , , . die doel van hierdie ondersoek is om vas te stel ofeggolalie en interaksiegedrag in outistiese kinders binne η neer individuele video-opnames van die interaksie van vier outistiese kinders met 'n bekende volwassene is gemaak. η kategoriese sisteem is ontwerp vir die beoordeling van die kinders se interaksiegedrag. bevindings dui daarop dat onmiddellikeeggolalie meer is as slegs die vtelslose herhaling van woorde. die uitinge is meestal relevant en dui op die oordrag van betekenisvolle informasie en die behoud van sotiale interaksie. gevolgtrekkings word gemaak oor die fasilitasie van kommunikasie deur die modifikasie en uitbreiding van onmiddellike eggolalie. of the language abnormalities which are quoted as primary criteria in the diagnosis of autism, immediate echolalia is the most frequently cited characteristic of autistic children who have some verbal expressive ability (prizant & duchan, 1982). echolalia is generally defined, by many authors (fay, 1980; hurtig et al., 1982; wing, 1976) as the meaningless and automatic production of words, without understanding, that are an exact or partial copy of those originally spoken by another person. i there is a certain controversy surrounding the significance of immediate echolalia for the autistic child. behaviourally orientated researches consider echolalia as an undesirable symptom of the language behaviour of autistic children (koegal et al., 1974). they consider echolalia as a communication disorder in itself and therefore advocate its extinction or replacement through the use of behaviour modification procedures (lovaas, 1977). i a recent trend in research has been to consider immediate echolalia in terms of how it may function for autistic children (prizant & duchan, 1982). fay (1973), suggests that immediate echolalia enables the autistic child to maintain social interaction in the face of a severe comprehension problem. philips & dyer (1977), hypothesize that immediate echolalia is a late onset form of normal imitative functioning in young children and that with the appropriate intervention programme, the echolalia becomes useful to the child and gives him access to an experience with the complexities of grammar. considering the diversity of views held in relation to immediate echolalia and how it may function for autistic children, it is of contention that these children, who have a severe language problem are attempting to communicate and interact socially with other people to the best of their limited capabilities. during the past few years (schuler, 1980) much work has been carried out on the pragmatic die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 aspects of language. emphasis has shifted away from a purely structural view toward a more functional approach. prizant & duchan (1982) analyzed immediate echoic utterances according to gaze behaviour and other non-verbal components such as general body orientation and manipulation of objects. seven distinct categories of echolalia were derived and a function was attributed to each structural category, namely: non-focussed; turn-taking; declarative; rehearsal; self-regulatory; yes-answer; and request function. however, a functional approach does have certain limitations in that it requires an interpretation of the autistic child's communication intent in a particular situation. contextual cues (e.g. the reaction of the listener) are frequently used as indicators of the function of a particular verbalization (dore, 1978). one is often inclined to interpret verbalizations according to one's own perspective. schuler (1980) emphasizes, that in judging the relevance of verbalizations one should guard against over-interpretation. these overinterpretations become even more tempting when observed speech behaviours resemble our own speech. consideration of the conflicting views of researchers in relation to the function of immediate echolalia for the autistic child and the limitations of a functional approach has led to this research. the main focus of this study is to determine whether immediate echolalia can function within a communication system. echoic and non-echoic utterances are viewed within a framework where the initiation of utterances, turn-taking, and the segmental features of the utterances are considered to determine the relevance of the autistic child's utterances; analyzing the echoic and non-echoic behaviour in natural communicative interactions considering situational factors and nonverbal behaviours co-occurring with the production of echoic utterances. © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 ingrid van zyl, ema alant and isabel uys method aims to determine whether the verba, utterances, in particular the immediate echoic responses of autistic children have communicative relevance as described by prizant and duchan (1982). to construct a categorical system whereby interactive behaviour and immediate echolalic responses of autistic children can be analyzed as relevant or irrelevant in communication. subjects a preliminary study was carried out by the examiner to select suitable subjects and to clarify the procedural aspects of the experiment. the 4 subjects who participated in the experiment were selected from the unica school for autistic children in pretoria. each subject had to be diagnosed as autistic by a professional evaluation team comprising two psychiatrists; a clinical psychologist; a speech clinician; the principal of the unica school and one teacher. the department of national education (1971) requires that each child admitted to the unica school must fulfil the four essential criteria of the autistic syndrome (rutter, 1974): — onset of the disorder prior to thirty months of age; — disturbances of language and communication; — persistent, ritualistic and compulsive behaviour; — disturbed social relationship each child must also display a minimum of seven of the 14 core symptoms noted by clancy et al. (1964) which are most persistently present in the syndrome of infantile autism and are regarded as the major manifestations of the disease. to qualify for participation in the experiment, each child had to demonstrate that at least 25 % of all verbal productions were echoic responses (prizant and duchan, 1982). study was on functional communication rather than the relationship between this and cognition. procedure during interaction with the children it was noted that they did not make contact with the examiner. therefore the resident speech clinician participated as the interlocutor in the experiment. the structured activities designed to elicit verbal responses from the children during the interaction with the interlocutor, were selected according to a preliminary study. the same activities were used for all four subjects. although this may seem inappropriate due to the wide age and mental status range, these activities were recommended by the resident speech clinician and the children's teachers, since eating and drinking activities are enjoyed by children of all ages. data collection was extended over a three day period for each child with a one day interval. each child was videotaped in interaction with the interlocutor on two occasions, each 30 minutes in length, to ensure that a reliable sample of the child's communicative abilities was obtained. the situation involved direct interaction between the interlocutor and the child. during the interaction the interlocutor was directed to interact/communicate with each child as naturally as possible and to indirectly encourage the child to communicate by eliciting the child's interest in the activities provided. analysis: categorical system a preliminary study indicated that the categorical system described by prizant & duchan (1982) was highly complex and subjective in the interpretation of the function of immediate echolalia. therefore the examiner constructed a categorical system for the purpose of analyzing the relevance and irrelevance of the children's utterances, including their echolalic responses based on interpretation table 1 criteria for subject selection criterion subject 1 subject 2 subject 3 subject 4 mental status griffiths mental development scales 24 months non-testable 29 months 36 months age 5:8 8:3 8:5 11:1 previous therapy none * — auditory perception receptive and expressive language stimulation * — auditory perception receptive and expressive language expansion none language afrikaans afrikaans afrikaans afrikaans * during speech therapy echoic utterances were accepted but not encouraged by the speech clinician. .table 1 presents a number of the criteria used during subject selection. the age of the subjects was not deemed a significant factor since the study was focussed on the relevance of the interactive behaviour and particularly, immediate echolalia, and not on the performance of a specific age group. although reference is made to the mental status of each subject, this too was not considered a significant factor. the focus of the of behavioural aspects co-occurring with the children's utterances (see table 2). x due to the complex nature of the categorization procedures, data analysis was restricted to 20 utterances in each interaction. the first 20 utterances of each interlocutor — child interaction were selected since the children displayed excellent co-operation and attention at the beginning of each interaction. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) immediate echolalia and the interactive behaviour of autistic children table 2 categorical system for analyzing the relevance of utterances 27 1 initiation:a spontaneous, original utterance created by the child and not an echoic response. 2. delayed echolalia: a repetition of stored utterances which the child has heard some time before, in a new and usually inappropriate context (fay, 1980). 3. turn-taking: smooth interchanges between the child and the interlocutor (prutting, 1982); each utterance in this category is also coded according to whether the turn was:verbal: indicating that the utterance is not accompanied by any non-verbal behaviour. verbal and non-verbal: indicating that the child's verbal response is accompanied by one or more of the following non-verbal behaviours: — eye contact with the interlocutor — gaze behaviour at the object, — head movements, — smiling, and — deliberate gestural actions; 4. relevance: the child's utterance is either designated as a self-directed or as an other-directed response; self-directed: a self-directed utterance is used by the child to monitor his action, or to focus control on his actions or used for planning what he will do next (tough, 1977). other-directed: an other-directed utterance is directed at the interlocutor by the child for demonstrating the actions he requests, instructing, forward planning, and anticipating collaborative action (tough, 1977). the relevance of the utterance is coded by a positive or a negative rating. a positive rating indicates that the child's utterance displays communicative intent. indicators of communicative intent: any of the following behaviour associated with the verbal response: eye contact with the interlocutor. according to argyle (1973), eyes provide crucial information about where the person is looking, and the area around the eyes is extremely expressive. one looks to obtain information and to send signals; this is all concerned with the flow of information needed to perform the social skills of interacting. appropriate gaze behaviour at the object. direction of gaze shows the direction of that person's attention (argyle, 1973). deliberate and voluntary actions. hand movements especially, play a role in social interaction. their principle function is as illustrators accompanying speech, and augmenting it when verbal skills are inadequate (argyle, 1973). the echoic response is functionally relevant to the task, or to the child's self-directed utterance, or to the interlocutor's utterance indicating communicative intent. a negative rating indicates that the child's response is irrelevant and a purely meaningless repetition; devoid of any indication of communicative intent. 5. linguistic segmental features: the utterance is rated according to whether the child changes or exactly repeats the linguistic segmental features of the model utterance. „ • ' changed: the child uses some of the interlocutor's words or either adds, deletes, or substitutes elements and is therefore not an exact repetition of the model utterance (prizant and duchan, 1982). same: the child gives an exact, complete or partial repetition of the interlocutor's model utterance. 6 non-segmental features: the non-segmental features of "the child's echoic response are rated according to the similarity in intonatioh, stress, speaking rate, duration, and voice to the model utterance. linguists recognize that timing, pitch and stress and other non-segmentals are integral to the meaning of utterances. changed: the child varies one or more of the non-segmental features of the model utterance. same: the child produces an exact repetition of the non-segmental features of the model utterance. in each subcategory, changed or the same, the echoic response is coded as being either a complete or an incomplete repetition:a complete repetition indicates that the child repeats the entire model utterance, an incomplete repetition indicates that the child echoes only a part of the model utterance. raters used to control observation the utterances were analyzed independently by the examiner and the resident speech clinician to objectify observations. after being trained on the significant features of the categorical system, the resident speech clinician viewed the first video-recording of interaction with each child. the interjudge reliability was measured by comparison of the results obtained by the examiner and the resident speech clinician. non-parametric statistical procedures were used due to the small number of subjects used in the experiment. results interjudge reliability non-parametric statistical procedures determined that a high agreement (97,34%) existed between the paired judgements of the examiner and the second judge, indicating that the categorical system designed by the examiner was a practical and easy to use scale. the reliability of this analysis procedure therefore seems to be high. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 ingrid van zyl, erna alant and isabel y s categorization of the utterances figure 1 represents a summary of the data obtained from the categorization of the autistic children's echoic and non-echoic utterances. it demonstrates a tendency for a greater occurrence of immediate echolalia (61,2%), followed by initiation (36,9%) and only a token usage of delayed echolalia (1,9%). there is a marked difference between verbal versus verbal and non-verbal turn-taking; verbal and non-verbal turns being the most prevalent (81,9%). it can be seen that other-directed utterances had a higher rate of occurrence (85%) than self-directed utterances (15%), but for both these categories most utterances were rated as relevant. it is interesting to note that 87,5% of the other-directed utterances were rated as relevant whereas only 66,7% of the self-directed utterances were relevant; suggesting (tentatively) that other-directed utterances are more likely to be rated as relevant than are self-directed utterances. e s 1 ζ 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 10 utterances turn-taking re1.evance j m 1 immediate initiation delayed verbal verbal & echolalia echolalia non-vertial 100 g 90 υ 8 80 2 ε £ 8 70 3 u 60 oc ο 60 8 1 50 η 2 ε η • β 40 •g υ ε £ ε 30 do 2 υ ο 20 i 3 10 £ linguistic segmental features non-segmental features changed same changed incomplete complete incomplete complete figure 1 categorization in percentages of the autistic subjects' echoic and non-echoic utterances. the data shows that the linguistic segmental features of the immediate echolalic utterances were mostly rated as the same (83,8%), rather than as changed (16,2%). the non-segmental features of the immediate echolalia were more often rated as changed with only an incomplete repetition of the model utterance (75%). the incidence of the use of the same non-segmental features with a complete repetition of the model utterance was minimal (3,5%). the data presented in figure 1 initiated the calculation of the infor'mation in figure 2. the relevance of the children's utterances is reviewed including a more specific analysis of the relevance of the immediate echolalia. echoic and non-echoic utterances viewed together had a higher incidence of being rated as relevant (84,4%) in comparison to only 15,6% of these utterances being rated as irrelevant. a more specific analysis of the immediate echolalia showed a large difference berelevanee of the utterances % relative to the total no of utterances relevance of the immediate echolalia % relative to total no of immediate echoic utterances subject 1 \z/a relevant i | irrelevant subject 2 subject 3 subject 4 figure 2 analysis of the relevance of the utterances tween the ratings of relevance and of irrelevance. of the immediate echolalia, 82,6% was relevant whereas only 17,5% was rated as irrelevant. specific analysis of each individual subject's performance was carried out and summarized into a single table (table 3) indicating jeach subject's performance as a percentage relative to the total number of utterances. a brief comparison of the subjects' performances according to the data in table 3 indicates that subject 4 has the highest initiation of original utterances (55%), followed by 40% immediate echolalia and only 5% delayed echolalia. of these utterances, 95% were rated as relevant and 100% of the immediate echolalia was rated as relevant. it is interesting to note that subject 4 had the highest mental status for all four subjects (36 months) and was the oldest subject. subject 2's performance shows very little initiation (17,5%), no delayed echolalia and a high incidence of immediate echolalia. subject 2 was, at the time of the study non-testable and mental status could not be determined. subject 1 and 3 display the same general tendencies of more initiation than delayed echolalia and the majority of utterances being immediate echolalia (55% and 67,5% respectively). they also show a high rate of relevant utterances and relevant immediate echolalia. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) immediate echolalia and the interactive behaviour of autistic children t&ble 3 performance of subjects on each category 29 4. initiation delayed echolalia turn-taking 3.1 verbal 3.2 verbal and non-verbal relevance 4.1 self-directed 4.2 other-directed + + linguistic segmental features 5.1 changed 5.2 same non-segmental features 6.1 changed — complete — incomplete 6.2 same — complete — incomplete immediate echolalia rating % relative to the total number of utterances subject 1 the range of the four subjects' performances indicates interesting tendencies as well as variance among their performances. in the category of initiation, subject 2 scored 17,5% whereas subject 4 scored 55%, pointing out that a large variance exists in the intersubject performance. this could1 possibly be related to the variance between these subjects' mental ages. the average of the 4 subjects' performances in initiation (36,9%) is therefore not indicative of the individual scores. a broader inspection of subject 2's and subject 4's overall performance shows that subject 2 has 82,5% immediate echolalia compared to subject 4's:40% rating; a difference of 42,5%. however, in the case of relevance both these subjects (2 and 4) have the highest percentage of utterances rated as relevant, that is 90% and 95% respectively. therefore the apparent difference in performance in production of the type of utterance does not necessarily affect the rating of relevance of the subjects' utterances. a relatively small range of performance exists between the four subjects' production of delayed echolalia, the highest score being 5% (subject 4) and the lowest rating 0% (subjects 2 and 3). the category of turn-taking indicates a wider range of performance among the subjects. subject 1 has 42,5% verbal turns compared to 57,5% verbal and non-verbal turns. the remaining subjects show a much greater difference between these two sub-categories with a higher incidence of verbal and non-verbal turns. for example, subject 2 has 2,5% verbal turns compared to 97,5% verbal and non-verbal turns. the data supports a general tendency of a greater percentage occurrence of verbal and non-verbal turns. in the category of relevance there is a small range between the subjects' performances indicating a general tendency for a greater prevadie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 42,5 2,5 42,5 57,5 7,5 2,5 70,0 20,0 subject 2 17,5 0 2,5 97,5 5,0 5,0 85,0 5,0 subject 3 32,5 0 7,5 92,5 20,0 10,0 55,0 15,0 subject 4 55,0 5,0 20,0 80,0 7,5 2,5 87,5 2,5 % relative to the individual total no of immediate echoic utterances 22,7 77,3 13,5 77,3 4,6 4,6 55,0 0 100,0 15.2 48,4 6,1 30.3 82,5 29,6 70,4 7,4 74,1 3,7 14,8 67,5 12,5 87,5 0 100,0 0 0 40,0 lence of other-directed utterances (85%) and the large majority of both self-directed and other-directed utterances being rated as relevant (84,4%). there is a degree of variance between the subjects' performances in the category of linguistic segmental features. subject 2 produced all his immediate echolalia with the same linguistic segmental features of the model utterances. subject 3 produced the greatest change in the linguistic segmental features (29,6%) of the model utterances. there is however a general tendency for the four subjects to display the same linguistic segmental features of the model utterance. characteristic for all the subjects is the general tendency for nonsegmental features of the immediate echoic utterances to be more often changed and incomplete repetitions of the model utterance. the percentage production of immediate echolalia differs among the subjects. subject 2 produced the most immediate echolalia (82,5%) whereas subject 4 produced the least immediate echolalia (40%), but both subjects have the highest ratings of relevance, that is 84,9% and 100% of their immediate echolalia was rated relevant respectively. subject 1 who produced the most verbal (only) turns (42,5%) produced the least relevant immediate echolalia (68,2%). however comparison of the individual subject's relevant and irrelevant immediate echolalia indicates a general tendency of a greater percentage occurrence of relevant immediate echolalia. discussion the general attitude toward autistic immediate echolalia is to view it as an automatic and meaningless repetition of words, and not as r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 ingrid van zyl, erna alant and isabel ljys the child's attempt to initiate and sustain social contact (fay, 1980) or to communicate his intentions. the high occurrence of immediate echolalia is coupled with the finding that 85% of these echoic responses were rated as relevant, meaningful and an attempt at communication within their limited capabilities. in response to, "gaan jy nou saamwerk?" subject 4 replied "saamwerk", while smiling and making rare eye contact with the interlocutor. subject 4 changed the non-segmental features of the model utterance and made a statement of the word he echoed and did not copy the questioning tones of the model utterance. he clearly displayed his intention to the interlocutor through the manipulation of an immediate echoic response accompanied by non-verbal cues of eye-contact and smiling. in the interaction analyzed by the examiner, the children always responded to a question or a statement directed at them by the interlocutor. acceptance and expansion of echoic behaviour appeared to reinforce the autistic child's turn-taking echoes and utterances. important non-verbal communication co-occurred with the otherdirected utterances providing indicators of intent whereby the utterances could be rated. these responses can be interpreted in terms of attention, need for interpersonal feedback and an attempt at sustaining the flow of interaction (argyle, 1973). a comparison of the individual subjects' performance shows some interesting differences. a brief comparison shows subject 4, the oldest subject producing the most initiation, less immediate echolalia and no delayed echolalia; whereas subject 2 displayed the least initiation, no delayed echolalia and the most immediate echolalia. this suggests that subject 4 has more advanced language skills as shown by his ability to produce more initiated utterances and fewer echoic responses supporting the view of carr, schreibman and lovaas (1975) that immediate echolalia may be an early stage in the development of normal language functioning. of great importance is the fact that a very small difference existed between these two subjects in relation to the relevance of their echoic and non-echoic utterances; subject 2 displayed 90,0% relevance and subject 4, 95,0% relevance. this suggests that the relevance of the autistic child's echoic and non-echoic utterances is not necessarily dependent on the child's level of speech and language development. subject 3 produced the greatest change in the linguistic segmental features of the model utterance, using some of the interlocutor's words and adding elements to indicate his own meaning. prizant and duchan (1982) view this type of immediate echolalia as probably the most intentional and least automatic response and an example of intentional mitigation. subject 3's performance may be influenced by the fact that he received speech therapy to expand receptive and expressive language and had a mental status of 29 months. the examiner recognizes that analysis of the child's utterances alone is a limitation of the study. analysis of the interlocutor's utterances would provide interesting and relevant information related to the type of response made by the child. however, a brief survey of the interlocutor's utterances shows that when the interlocutor repeated a child's relevant immediate echoic response and the child echoed the interlocutor, the child's second echo was more likely to be rated "as irrelevant. this suggests that the speech clinician should take care not to inhibit the child's relevant utterances by producing meaningless repetitions of the child's echoes. implications for diagnosis and treatment the findings of this study should be regarded within the greater framework of diagnosis and treatment of the autistic child. as previously cited, behaviourally orientated researchers (koegal, lovaas and schreibman, 1974) consider echolalia a communication disorder and advocate the extinction or replacement of echolalic behaviours through the use of behaviour modification procedures (lovaas, 1977) the findings of this study dispute a non-functional view of immediate echolalia. the consideration of immediate echolalia as having communicative intent must form an integral part of diagnosis and therapy. the clinician should guard against regarding a high occurrence of echoic utterances in the child as indicating less meaning than a higher occurrence of non-echoic utterances, but should rather view echolalia as possibly a necessary stage of language development for verbal autistic children (philips and dyer, 1977). according to prizant & duchan (1982), indiscriminate extinction of all forms of immediate echolalia is ill-advised because of the functions that echolalia may serve for autistic children. individual differences do exist and each child has a certain potential to learn to communicate. different therapeutic procedures should be used to treat, for example, a child who has been predominantly echolalic for some years than those used to treat a child who displays comprehension and communicative intent with the echoic utterances he produces. the latter child needs to accept and exploit immediate echolalia and learn to relate the repetitions to aspects of the environment and communicative interactions (prizant & duchan, 1982). references argyle, m. the psychology of interpersonal behaviour. penguin books ltd, harmondsworth, great britain, 1973. argyle, m. social interaction. tavistock publications ltd, london, great britain, 1973. carr, e., et al. control of echolalic speech in psychotic speech. j. abnor. child psycho., 3, 331-338, 1975. clancy, h., et al. the diagnosis of infantile austism. develop. med. child neuro., 11, 432, 1969. de c. murray, c.h., a report of the committee of enquiry into the treatment, education and care of autistic children. department of national education, government print, pretoria, rsa, r.p. 26/72, 1971. dore, j. requestive, systematic nursery school conversation. in: campbell, r. and smith, r., (eds.), recent advances in the psychology of language. plenum press, new york, 1978. fay, w.h. on the echolalia of the blind and of the autistic child. j. speech hear. disord., 38, 478-489, 1973. i fay, w.h. aspects of language. in: fay, w.h. and schuler, a.l., (eds.) emerging language in autistic children. language intervention series, vol 5, edward arnold ltd, london, great britain, 1980. hurtig, r., ensrud, p, and tomelin, b. the communicative function of question production in autistic children. j. autism develop. disord., 12, 57-69, 1982. koegal, r., lovaas, o.i. and schreibman, l. a behaviour modification approach to the treatment of autistic children. j. autism child. schizoprenia. 4, 111-116, 1974. lovaas, o. the autistic child: language development through behaviour modification. halstead press, new york·;' 1977. philips, g. and dyer, c. late onset echolalia in autism and allied disorders. br. j. disord. commun., 12, 47-59, 1977. prizant, b.m. and duchan, j.f. the functions of immediate echolalia in austistic children. j. speech-hear. disord., 47, 241-249, 1982. prutting, c.a. pragmatics as social competence. j. speech hear. disord., al, 123-134, 1982. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy programme with spastic dysphonia a single case study 31 rutter, m. the development of infantile autism. j. psychol. med. 9, 147-163, 1974. schuler, a.l. aspects of communication. in: fay, w.h. and schuler, a.l., emerging language in autistic children. language intervention series, 5, edward arnold ltd, london, great britain, 1980. tough, j. the development of meaning: a study of children's use of language. george allen and unwin ltd, london, great britain, 1977. wing, l. early childhood autism: clinical, educational and social aspects. a wheaten and co., exeter, great britain, 1976. a stuttering therapy programme with spastic dysphonia a single case study ingrid meyers ba (sp & η th) (witwatersrand) speech therapy department baragwanath hospital denise anderson ba (sp & η th) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract this study was motivated by reported similarities in vocal tract dynamics in stuttering and spastic dysphonia. the effects of a stuttering therapy programme with an adult with spastic dysphonia were observed. subjective and objective measures obtained preand posttherapeutically included a qualitative analysis, laryngographic tracings, and fiberoptic examinations. results showed subtle improvements on all measures suggesting improved laryngeal behaviours. findings are discussed in relation to therapeutic utility. opsomming die motivering vir hierdie ondersoek is gebasseer op die ooreenkomste t.o.v. die dinamiek van die vokale gang tydens hakkel en spastiese disfonie dieuitwerkingvan 'nterapieprogram vir hakkel wat uitgevoer is op 'n pasient met spastiese disfonie, is waargeneem. subjektiewe en objektiewe metings is vooren na-terapeuties van laringografiese afdrukke, fiberoptiese ondersoeke en van 'n kwalitatiewe anahse verkry. resultate dui op 'n subtiele verbetering op alle metings, wat verbeterde laringale werking aandui. die bevindinge word bespreek t. o. v. kliniese bruikbaarheid. of all the human voice disorders, the syndrome of adductor spastic dysphonia has remained the most mysterious, the most poorly understood, and the most resistant to effective treatment, (boone 1972; aronson 1980; reich and till 1983). spastic dysphonia is a rare disorder and literature on the subject is limited, which has led to some confusion with regard to treatment (wolk, 1980). yet most authorities agree on its poor response to therapy procedures. the disorder, first described by traube in 1871 is characterized by "a strained, creaking, choked vocal attack and a tense squeezed voice accompanied by extreme tension of the entire phonatory system." (luchsinger and arnold 1965). various terms have been used to describe the disorder of spastic dysphonia. it has been referred to as 'glottal spasms', 'stammering of the vocal cords' and 'laryngeal stuttering', and has been parallelled to the disorder of stuttering by mccall (1975) and salamy and sessions (1980). stuttering is a more common disorder, and the age of onset and sex distribution are well defined. the two disorders can, however be likened in terms of their variability, development and course, etiological controversy and history of therapeutic failure with high rates of symptom relapse (ingham and andrews 1973; aronson 1980; salamy and sessions 1980). perhaps the abnormality in the physiology of the larynx for stuttering and spastic dysphonia could be considered a major similarity. mccall (1975) found that similarities between spastic dysphonia and the stuttering block included muscle spasms, disturbed muscle tone and involuntary movements e.g. tremor, with normal laryngeal structures evident on laryngologic examination. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 the past decade has brought about a dramatic transition in the theoretical foundation and purpose in the treatment of stuttering. based on the factors which induce fluency in stutterers, remediation has shifted to manipulation of phonation utilizing behaviour modification techniques as a vehicle for establishing fluency (shames and florance 1980). common to all 'fluency-based' programmes are smooth initiation of phonation, decreased rate and continuous phonation and breath flow which facilitates co-ordination of the vocal folds concurrent with the execution of articulatory gestures (miller 1982). schwartz (1976) felt that it was the reduction in stress on the cords which facilitated their laxing and lengthening and enabled their greater bulk to vibrate. the literature on spastic dysphonia does not reflect the same advances regarding treatment approaches. while some authorities have described similar techniques to those constituting 'fluency-based' programmes, no-one has as yet integrated these techniques into a unified therapy programme. in view of the limited investigations into the field of spastic dysphonia, and of the need to explore new avenues for treatment, the writers decided to investigate the effects of a fluency-based programme (developed for stutterers) by shames and florance (1980) on a subject with spastic dysphonia. method subject the subject, s, was an english speaking adult male, aged 34 years whose profession involved much public speaking. his voice problem © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an investigation of non-standard english syntax in 12-year old c o l o u r e d children karen c. malan ( b . s c . logopaedics, u . c . t . ) tygerberg hospital, cape town summary non-standard english (nse) syntactic constructions occurring in the natural spdech, behaviour of a group of 20 12-year old coloured children were identified and analysed. three syntactic classes were isolated as having contained nse constructions used significantly by the group: auxiliary/copula verb forms, number-verb agreement and tense. a bi-dialectal sentence repetition task was then adminstered to the same group and to a matched group of white children, in which the two groups were compared on their ability to reproduce sentences containing (1) nse constructions used significantly by the coloured group, and (2) the standard english (se) forms of these constructions. results indicated that while coloured ss were able to reproduce many of the se constructions, their overall performance on these items was significantly inferior to that of white ss. conversely, their repetition of nse items was significantly superior to that of white ss. the implications of these findings for the clinical language assessment of coloured children were discussed. opsomming die voorkoms van nie-standaard engelse (nse) sintaktiese konstruksies in die natuurlike spraakgedrag van 'n groep van 20 twaalfjarige kleurlingkinders is gei'dentifiseer en geanaliseer. drie sintaktiese kategoriee bevattende nse konstruksies wat betekenisvol deur die groep gebruik is, is gei'soleer: hulpwerkwoord/kopula werkwoordvorme, werkwoord-getal ooreenstemming en tydsvorme. 'n sinsherhalingtaak is vervolgens in twee dialekte aan dieselfde, sowel as 'n soortgelyke blanke groep kinders, gestel. die twee groepe is vergelyk t.o.v. hulle vermoe om sinne bevattende (1) nse konstruksies en (2) standaard engelse (se) vorme van bogenoemde konstruksies, te produseer. resultate het getoon dat terwyl die kleurling groep instaat was om se konstruksie te produseer, hulle algemene prestasie t.o.v. hierdie items, betekenisvol swakker was as di6 van die blanke groep. die kleurling groep se herhaling van nse items was egter betekenisvol beter, as die van die blanke groep. die implikasies van die bevindinge t.o.v. die kliniese taalevaluasie van kleurlingkinders word bespreek. recent sociolinguistic research into the language differences of children from economically disadvantaged and culturally-different sub-communities has necessitated a re-evaluation of current approaches to the assessment and diagnosis of language 'disorders in these children. while procedures for the identification and treatment of language deviance and delay are well described in the literature, there is evidence of increasing concern among speech and language professionals that these procedures may be inappropriate, or at least inadequate, where the child concerned is one from a dialectal sub-community whose language manifests certain phonological, lexical or syntactic differences from that of ' the standard language of the larger community1' 1 4 ' 1 5 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) non-standard syntax of coloured children 69 two major factors have formed the basis for this concern. 1. firstly, there is a lack of documented information on the language norms prevailing in many linguistic sub-communities. without this knowledge, it is difficult to differentiate between socially induced language differences and true language pathology. 2. secondly, the commonly-used measures of linguistic behaviour are based on the norms of a "standard" language, and hence run the risk of indicating language deviance or delay in a child who may simply be using rules which conform to his own dialect. research in the field of sociolinguistics provides some indications as to how these shortcomings might be overcome. in particular, the notions of socio-cultural constraints on language use and "communicative competence"9 have implications for both the sampling of language data and assessment of the dialectally-different child. recent advances in the study of language as a social phenomenon have indicated that an adequate structural description of a speaker's language cannot be provided without consideration of the effects of socio-cultural factors on the speaker's language use. 6 ' 7 ' 9 ' 1 0 ' 1 6 these include such factors as the attitude of speakers toward their language varieties, the roles and status of speakers, the relative formality of the situation or topic of the message, all of which may have a systematic effect on a speaker's linguistic competence in that they will affect his choice of speech style or his use of one particular syntactic form over another. evidence of systematic variation in language use as a function of contingencies in the linguistic and social environment has led to the suggestion that it is part of a speaker's knowledge or competence of his language that he "knows" implicitly which linguistic rules to apply in which social contexts. this in turn has led to a reformulation of the traditional, chomskian notion of linguistic competence to a more broadly defined "communicative competence" which includes not only a speaker's knowledge of the rules of grammar, but also the ability or competence for the appropriate uses of language in different contexts.3· 9 · 1 0 the integral role of socio-cultural factors in language use afforded by this definition has important implications for the sampling of linguistic data from dialectally-different children. it makes clear that the type of speech behaviour obtained in sampling will vary greatly according to the setting and circumstances in which it is elicited. it means that in order to obtain reliable data concerning the natural speech behaviour of children, the language clinician must know a great deal about the contextual variables which govern speech production and control for them accordingly. the notion of competence for use of language has further implications for the use with dialectally different children of assessment tests based on elicted imitation (ei). more specifically, it has helped to explain the differences in performance found between children from different dialectal communities on these tasks. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 karen malan sentence repetition or ei tasks have been widely used by investigators of child language and speech clinicians alike as a means of determining the capacity in young children developing language, for comprehension and expression.8' 1 7 • 1 3 they are based on the premise that a child will correctly reproduce only those sentence structures which are a part of his linguistic competence: 1 7 where an item is beyond his level of competence, the child is said to filter the sentence he hears through his own productive system, to reproduce it using the rules he knows.5 sociolinguists who adopted ei tasks as a source of information on the language behaviours of non-standard english (nse) speakers, found evidence that a similar type of "rule-filtering" occurred with much older (adolescent) speakers of nse dialects.1 0' 1 3 l a b o v 1 0 and baratz,2 in separate studies, found that when adolescent black nse speakers in the united states were given standard english (se) sentences to repeat, the majority tended to re-encode the sentences into nse form in their responses. thus, for example, given the sentence "i asked alvin if he knows how to play basketball", the majority of speakers repeated it as "i asks alvin do he know how to play basketball". 1 0 in terms of hymes 9 notion of communicative competence, these speakers might be said to have hacf the tacit knowledge for the se rule, in that the meaning of the sentence was preserved in their responses, yet did not show the ability or competence for use of the se construction. furthermore, baratz's2 findings indicated that white english speakers were significantly superior to black speakers in repeating se sentences, while black speakers were far superior to whites in repeating nse constructions. it was concluded from these findings that the language assessment of nse-speaking children should include measures of their knowledge of nse in addition to their knowledge of se. however, current language tests based on the ei technique, including the carrow elicited language inventory5 and the northwestern syntax screening t e s t , 1 1 fail to account for the influence of dialectal differences on performance. they are based solely on se norms, and hence are liable to score grammatical complexity as being absent when in fact it may be present, but its dialect is not that of the test instrument.1 it was the contention of the present study that an effective approach to the language assessment of the dialectally-different child requires: 1. knowledge of the linguistic forms used in the dialect of the child's community, and v •*' 2. the availability of assessment tools which are sensitive to the dialectal variations present in the child's language. the need to meet these requirements was seen to be particularly relevant to the language assessment of children from the englishspeaking "coloured" community of cape t o w n 1 8 . that the speech of this group is characterized by certain phonological, lexical and grammatical differences is clearly apparent to the outside observer. yet the lack of documented information on its linguistic forms1 4 make ! the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) non-standard syntax of coloured children 71 it unclear as to whether there can be said to be a distinct pattern of the language spoken in this community, while at the same time, our measures of language proficiency are based on the norms of a white, middle-class, "standard" english speaking population, and hence of questionable validity to this group. this study attempted a partial fulfillment of these needs, by investigating the presence of nse syntactical constructions in, the language of coloured children, and the implications of these for the use with the coloured child of one type of assessment tool — viz. test based' on elicited imitation. m e t h o d a i m s 1. the elicitation and analysis of samples of natural speech behaviour from a group of 12 year old english-speaking coloured children, to identify the significant presence and/or trends of nse language patterns. 2. administration of a bi-dialectal sentence repetition task to the same group of coloured children and a matched group of white children, in which the two groups are compared on their ability to reproduce sentences containing: (a) nse syntactic constructions used significantly by the coloured group, and (b) the se forms of these constructions.s u b j e c t s part i: ss comprised a group of 20 coloured school-children, of which 10 were boys and 10 girls. ages ranged from 12.0 to 13.0 years. all ss came from english-speaking homes and attended english-medium classes at their schools. all were from low socio-economic status families, as determined by parental occupation. all were judged by their teachers to possess average intelligence and normal speech, hearing and language abilities. ss were representative of 2 broad areas of the cape peninsula. part ii: a matched group of 20 english-speaking white schoolchildren, judged to be representative of white south african sespeaking children, was added to the experimental population. these were matched to the first group by applying the same criteria with regard to age, sex, home-language, intelligence and speech, hearing and language abilities. parental occupations fell within the range of the middle socio-economic group. p r o c e d u r e part i: language samples were obtained from each of the 20 coloured ss in recorded sessions, in which ss were interviewed by a coloured die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 karen malan experimenter (e). language was elicited in 3 stimulus situations: 1. unstructured conversation with the e. 2. responses to a set of questions related to 5 stimulus photographs. to ensure consistency in the method of eliciting language and to ensure a minimum number of utterances from each s, five directions were supplied for each of the 5 enlarged photographs. these were constructed (after l e o n a r d 1 2 ) so as to elicit present, future, past and future conditional tenses. 3. conversation with the ε on 4 topics, each generated by a prescribed set of questions. each session was recorded onto tape by the writer. language samples from each child were transcribed for the purpose of isolating nse constructions used by each s. these were then categorized according to grammatical class for examination of possible trends of nse usage. a manipulation of the chi-square formula was used to determine which nse constructions were used significantly by the group as a whole. these comprised the "critical constructions" to be used in part ii of the study. part ii: for each of the 'critical' nse constructions, a nse sentence containing that construction was devised, as well as a corresponding se sentence containing the standard form of the construction. these sentences, randomized for presentation, formed the basis of the sentence repetition,'task administered approximately 2 weeks later to the same group of '''coloured" ss as well as the control group of white ss. the task was administered to the coloured group by the same coloured ε used in part i, while the writer served as ε for the white group. ss were seen individually by the es. responses were recorded in writing immediately, as well as tape recorded, to ensure accuracy. repetitions were scored in two categories according to whether the critical construction of a sentence was "repeated verbatim" by the s or "altered to opposite dialect". chi-square analyses were used to determine for each group, which se and nse sentences were "repeated verbatim" by a significant number of ss in the group, and which were "altered to opposite dialect". also, chi-square analyses were used to compare the responses of the two groups to se and nse sentences. t-tests for independent samples were used to determine whether a significant difference existed between the two groups in the total number of verbatim repetitions of (1) se and (2) nse sentences. / • /' i results and discussion i part i using the chi-square formula to (determine the frequency of occurrence of nse constructions which would be necessary to yield a significant chi-square value, it was determined that above 14 (p < the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) non-standard syntax of coloured children 73 0.05), nse constructions were used by a significant number of ss. the 7 constructions which fell above this point were: (1) absence of the auxiliary " a r e " . (2) absence of copula " a r e " . (3) absence of auxiliary "have". (4) absence of 3rd person singular present tense marker (-s) (5) absence of regular past marker (-ed). (6) simple present replacing future conditional tense. (7) simple future replacing future conditional tense. these constructions were grouped together with the remaining nse constructions in context of the syntactic class to which they belonged. a total of 9 syntactic classes were found to contain nse constructions; for purposes of the present article, however, only those classes containing nse forms used significantly by the group (the "critical constructions") are presented. auxiliary/copula verb forms. certain forms of 'be' and ' h a v e ' — " a r e , " "has" and "have" — were deleted in present tense constructions (see table i). a single tendency may underlie all of the items listed in table i, in that absence of auxiliary/copula forms occurred only for those forms of 'be' and ' h a v e ' which in se are contractible. the se contraction rule removes all but the final segments of these auxiliaries in present tense forms. it seems that coloured speakers may be applying the contraction rule, and then applying a further rule for deletion of the segments remaining after contraction. furthermore, deletion occurred most frequently in instances where the preceding subject was a pronoun. taken together, these factors suggest that where contraction is expected in se, coloured dialect may delete, and that the probability of deletion is greater when the subject is a pronoun. table i: non-standard auxiliary/copula verb forms in coloured children no. non-standard english construction example no. of ss i absence of forms "to be"· (1) + auxiliary "are" in present progressive forms (are + -ing) (2) + copula "are" in simple present (s-cop.-o) they fighting we five in the family 20 16 ii absence of forms "to have" (1) auxiliary have in present perfect " h a s " + past participle η—"have" + past participle she got a pencil in her hand i got two uncles 3 18 + indicates nse constructions used significantly by the group. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 karen malan number-verb agreement nse forms in this category are listed in table ii. these forms may all be indicative of a single tendency among ss to eliminate number distinction in verbs and demonstrative pronouns. this tendency appears to be a simplifying process, but may be directly related to the influence of afrikaans syntax: (1) there are no inflected verb-endings for the 3rd person singular in afrikaans; hence the same verb form is used for all persons. by eliminating 3rd person singular present tense markers, coloured english speakers achieve a similar simplicity of form. (2) ss use of english "is" and "was" regardless of number, reflects a similar pattern to afrikaans where there is only one present tense form of the verb "to be" ("is") and one past tense form ("was"). (3) the use of singular demonstratives with plural nouns may be similarly explained, since there is no plural form for demonstratives in afrikaans. table ii: non-standard number-verb agreement in coloured children no. non-standard english construction example no. of ss i absence of 3rd person singular present tense markers: (1) + /s/ suffix (2) "has" and "does" my mommy say she think . . . it just have to be like that 18 7 ii plural subject takes singular form of "be": (1) present tense (2) past tense (3) "there + be + plural np subject their clothes is dirty the cops was fighting there is leaves on the floor 8 2 13 iii plural noun takes singular demonstrative one of that stones 6 tense (1) present tense forms were used pervasively, and replaced several more complex tense constructions. in particular, the simple present replaced past, future land future conditional tenses. it seems that coloured english speakers tend to omit the morphological endings which mark the contracted forms of these tenses (-ed, '11, a n d ' d respectively). it should not be inferred from this tendency, however, that ss we're insensitive to past, future and conditional tenses, since instances were found in the speech of the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) non-standard syntax of coloured children 75 nearly every s where the full forms of these tenses were used. furthermore, where reduction to the present tense occurred, it may have been directly related to phonological environment: for example, formation of the regular past in se involves simply the addition to the verb base of /t/ or id/, both sounds which tend to be lost in certain kinds of word-final consonant clusters, whether part of a past tense formation or n o t . 1 6 (2) the simple future was frequently used in place of the future conditional tense — possibly indicative of a preference to use a more tangible form of the verb, rather than the abstract conditional construction. table iii: non-standard tense usage in coloured children no. non-standard english construction example no. of ss 1 simple present tense replaces: e: what did you do? 17 (1) + simple past e: what did you do? 17 (1) + simple past s: i clean our pigeonhok (2) + future conditional e: what would you like 16 (2) + future conditional to do when you grow up? s: i like to travel around the world (3) simple future e: what will happen 13 (3) simple future next? s: the people buy it from them 11 simple future replaces: + future conditional e: what would you do? s: i'll take my friend's part 19 numerous further nse constructions were found in the categories of negation, question forms, adverbials, adjectives, pronouns and prepositions. since none of these categories contained nse forms used by a statistically significant number of ss, they have been omitted from discussion for purposes of the present article. from the preceding discussion, it seems evident that the nse forms produced by coloured ss cannot be considered without reference to the structural linguistic and socio-cultural factors which may have entered into their realization. it was seen that many of the nse forms might be considered the result of grammatical "interference" from afrikaans, to which the ss under study can be assumed to have had considerable exposure, afrikaans being the dominant language of the coloured community. the possible influence of phonological environment on certain of the forms was also referred to. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 karen malan a third consideration was that certain nse forms might be seen as reductions to a simpler form. certain of the nse constructions coincide at least in superficial form to stages in the language development of children in a se community. this is true of the forms used by ss comprising morpheme omission — omissions which are also found, interestingly, in many american nse dialects. brown4, however, has stressed that it should not be concluded from this that these forms constitute "immature" versions of se. he proposes that many morphemes, by virtue of the fact that they are frequently redundant to the meaning of an utterance, may be especially vulnerable to deletion. he finds no evidence that the 'misconstructions' of certain dialects are simpler than the standard constructions, merely that they are different. finally, it was evident from the preceding linguistic analysis that the use of nse forms by ss was not categorical. thus, for nearly every nse construction used, instances could be found in which the se form of that construction was used. for this reason, the syntactical differences shown by coloured ss should not be taken to indicate the consistent presence or absence of certain features, but might rather be viewed as a matter of differences in relative usage. in this regard, it should be noted that certain of the nse forms outlined above may be detected in the colloquial speech of many white south african english speakers (a case in point being the omission of auxiliary/copula "are"). it seems probable that there are social constraints which govern the probability of occurrence of nse forms in white south african english as opposed to coloured english. it is likely that the prestige value attached by white speakers to the standard form of the language serves to constrain their use of non-standard variants except in their most casual, unmonitored speech. and equally likely that the more a coloured speaker strives towards the values of the highest status societal group, the more his language will tend towards the standard variety. these factors would make it doubtful as to whether a standard pattern of non-standard usage could be said to exist in the coloured community. the variability shown by ss in this study in their use of nse forms appears to support this view. it is suggested, therefore, that rather than forming a separate dialect with distinct nse forms, so-called coloured english might be seen as part of a spectrum of south african english, which has at its one extreme white, middle-class se, and at the other extreme the dialectal variations of the coloured lower-class. in this view, coloured english forms part of the same continuum as se, sharing the vast majority of its rules, yet containing certain extensions and modifications of these rules which may be variably applied depending on factors in the linguistic and social context. , part ii i results of statistical analyses on the sentence repetition task are the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) non-standard syntax of coloured children 77 discussed in relation to the following questions which this part of the study aimed to explore: would coloured ss reproduce sentences containing se constructions used irregularly by them? or would they produce sentences conforming to the nse syntactical patterns identified in part i? results of chi-square analyses indicated that coloured ss showed considerable facility at reproducing verbatim many of the se constructions. it seems likely that the relative formality of the testing situation in this part of the study, as well as the nature of the task itself, both of which were conducive to a more careful and monitored style of speech, may have served to favour application of the standard rule in this task for some ss. in spite of these constraints, however, some proportion of coloured ss showed a tendency to re-encode each of the se constructions into nse form in their responses — indicating that they had decoded the sentence, then re-encoded it in the form they might have used in formulating the sentence themselves. would the repetition of certain nse forms be restricted to coloured ss, or might white ss show a similar tendency to produce nse forms? results indicated that, in general, when given se sentences, white ss repeated them verbatim, and that when given nse sentences, they re-encoded the sentence in terms of their own se rules to produce it in se form. however, given the constructions "auxiliary ' a r e ' " and "copula ' a r e ' " , in either se or nse, white ss showed some tendency to apply the nse rules in their responses. although this tendency was not statistically significant in either case, it seems that the rules for auxiliary and copula deletion are not necessarily peculiar to the speech of coloured children, but may be a part of the white child's linguistic behaviour as well. would the ss in each group be consistent in their "translations" from the unfamiliar dialect? examination of responses by each s in the two groups to se and nse sentences indicated that ss from both groups were consistent in their responses, in that when they translated a given sentence to their own dialect, all applied the same se or nse rule in their responses. this result appears to provide evidence for the validity of nse forms as representing a structured, yet different set of rules. if the nse forms used by coloured ss were a matter of random, unordered variation, one would not have expected consistency among the nse responses. could one group be said to be significantly superior to the other at repeating (1) se and (2) nse sentences? results of t-tests used to compare the total number of verbatim repetitions by each group to each of the 2 sentence types revealed that coloured ss were significantly superior to whites at reproducing correctly nse items, and that white ss were significantly superior to coloured ss in reproducing se sentences. the main trends of the results of part ii may be summarized as follows. while coloured ss showed considerable facility at reproducing die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 karen malan se forms which they had used irregularly in their spontaneous speech, their overall performance on se items was significantly inferior to that of white children. this was due to the fact that for each se item, a proportion of coloured ss reformulated the item to produce the nse rule in their output. that these reformulations showed a consistent pattern across all ss appears to provide evidence of the structured, rule-governed nature of the nse forms. furthermore, the performance of coloured ss was significantly superior to that of white ss when given nse sentences to reproduce, while conversely, their performance on se items was significantly inferior to that of white ss. while these findings must be considered preliminary, the indication is that coloured children may perform interiorly to white children on ei tests which have se as their criterion of correctness. the results would appear to justify the inclusion in current ei tests of measures of the clinical coloured child's ability to reproduce nse syntactic forms in addition to se forms. if a coloured child suspected of having a language disorder failed to reproduce a se construction, yet correctly reproduced the nse form of this construction, and if his alterations were similar to those of his age-peers within his community, we might assume that he is not language delayed/deviant, but using a wellordered though different rule. conclusion the analysis of speech samples obtained from coloured ss revealed the presence of numerous nse syntactic forms in their natural speech behaviour of which those involving auxiliary/copula verb forms, number-verb agreement, and tense were predominant. examination of this data indicated that the use of nse forms was not consistent, but rather a matter of relative usage of certain features, the occurrence of which could not be considered without reference to both structural (linguistic and phonological) and social contextual determinants. the second stage of the study was concerned with the question of whether the nse forms identified in the speech of coloured ss might produce differential effects on their performance in ei tasks as compared with the performance of white children. .results of a bi-dialectal sentence repetition task administered to white and coloured ss indicated that the overall performance of coloured ss on se items was significantly inferior to that of white ss, while their performance on nse items was significantly superior to that of white ss. these findings led to a suggestion for the reformulation of currently used ei tests in the clinical situation, to include additional measures of the coloured child's ability to reproduce nse syntactical constructions. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) non-standard syntax of coloured children 79 references 1. adler, s. (1979): poverty children and their language: implications for teaching and treating. new york: grune and stratton inc. 2. baratz, j. (1969): a bi-dialectal task for determining language proficiency in economically disadvantaged negro children. child development 40, 889-901. 3. bloom, l. & lahey, m. (1978): language development and language disorders. new york: wiley. 4. brown, r. (1973): a first language: the early stages. penguin books. 5. carrow, e. (1974): carrow elicited language inventory. austen, texas: learning concepts. 6 cazden, c. b. (1972): the situation: a neglected source of social class differences in language use. in j. b. pride and j. holmes (eds) sociolinguistics. penguin books. 7. cazden, c. b. (1972): child language and education. new york: holt, rinehart & winston, inc. 8. ervin, s. m. (1964): imitation and structural change in children's language. in ε. h. lenneberg (ed) new directions in the study of language. cambridge, mass.: m.i.t. press. 9 hymes, d. (1972): competence and performance in linguistic theory. in j. b. pride & j. holmes (eds) sociolinguistics. penguin books. 10 labov, w. (1972): language in the inner city: studies in the black english vernacular. philadelphia: university of pennsylvania press. 11. lee, l. (1969): northwestern syntax screening test. evanston: northwestern university press. 12. leonard, l. b. (1972): what is deviant language? journal of speech and hearing disorders 37(4), 427-446. 13. menyuk, p. (1971): the acquisition and development of language. englewood cliffs, n. j.: prentice-hall inc. 14. penn, c. (1978): speech pathology and audiology in south africa — past, present and future perspectives. in l. w. lanham and k. p. prinsloo (eds) language and communication studies in south africa. cape town: oxford university press. 15. severson, r. a. & guest, κ. e. (1970): toward the standardized assessment of the language of disadvantaged children. in f. williams (ed) language and poverty: perspectives on a theme. chicago: markham publishing co. 16. shuy, r. w. (1972): language problems of disadvantaged children. in j. w. irwin and m. marge (eds) principles of childhood language disabilities. englewood cliffs, n. j.: prentice-hall inc. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 karen malan 17. slobin, d. i. & welsh, c. a . (1973): elicited imitation as a research tool in developmental psycholinguistics. in c. a . ferguson and d. i. slobin (eds) studies of child language development. new york: holt, rinehart & winston inc. 18. whisson, m. g. (1971): the coloured people. abe bailey institute of international studies, university of cape town, rondebosch. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 49 vokaalreels by dowes santie ε meyer department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand opsomming alhoewel die bepaling van reelmatighede in spraakproduksie belangrik vir terapiebeplanning is, is dit nie altyd identifiseerbaar in die spraak van die dowe nie. die doel van die studie was om te bepaal of die normaalhorende luisteraar in staat is om die dowe se spraakreels te identifiseer. sewe dowes met 'n kongenitale, sensoriesneurale gehoorverlies het ses toetsvokale in woorde vyfkeer herhaal wat deur derdejaarstudente in oudiologie in 'n geslote-keuseformaat beoordeel is. twee luisteraars moes saamstem in 70% van die beoordelings dat 'n spesifieke vokaalgehoor is. uit die beoordelingsmatrikse blyk dit dat die luisteraars wel in staat was om konstanthede te identifiseer. die interluisteraarkorrelasies wissel van r=0.49 tot 0.89, wat impliseer dat die luisteraars nie altyd saamstem in hulle beoordelings van 'n spesifieke vokaal nie. die bevinding het implikasies vir die oudioloog in terme van spraakevalusie. abstract although the establishment of patterns in speech production is important for therapy planning they are not always identifiable in the speech of the deaf. in this study the object was to ascertain whether the normal hearing person was capable of identifying rules of speech. seven deaf speakers with a congenital, sensorineural hearing loss repeated six test words five times which were rated by third year audiology students in a closed set format. two listeners were required to agree in 70 % of the cases that a specific vowel was heard. the rating matrixes showed that the listeners were able to identify consistencies. the interlistener correlations varied from r=0.49 to 0.98, implying that the listeners did not always agree in their judgements regarding a specific vowel. in'terms of speech evaluation the findings pose implications for the audiologist. word. spraakreels ontwikkel waarskynlik by die gehoorgestremde (daar word hier spesifiek na die persoon met 'n binourale kongenitale ernstige tot totale sensoriesneurale gehoorverlies verwys) op dieselfde wyse as by die normaalhorende. die omgewingsleidrade wat deur die gehoorgestremde gebruik word, sal waarskynlik van die normaalhorende verskil in die sin dat visuele en slegs gedeeltelike ouditiewe inligting (meestal lae frekwensieinligting) beskikbaar en dus benutbaar is (boothroyd, 1978). daar kan verder verwag word dat die gehoorgestremde se reseptiewe leidraadsisteem uitgebrei en gemodifiseer sal word soos in die geval van die normaalhorende kind, afhangende van die gehoorgestremde se omgewing en sy vermoe om spraak uit die omgewing, te gebruik (fry, 1978). hy ontwikkel dus 'n sisteem wat as konstanthede of reelmatighede in sy spraak gei'dentifiseer kan word, ofte wel spraakreels. dit is egter 'n kontensieuse hipotese want daar is navorsers wat beweer dat daar nie sprake van 'n reelgebaseerde sisteem in die dowe se spraak is nie as gevolg van die hoe mate van variasie in produksie (harris et al., 1985). hierteenoor beweer ander outoriteite dat daar nie bewys kan word dat die uitings op 'n lukraak wyse en dus sonder enige plan, geskied nie (monsen, 1974; ten spyte van uitgebreide navorsing en verbetering in die spraakopleidingstegnologie word die spraak van die dowe steeds gekenmerk deur swak verstaanbaarheid (gold, 1980). nogtans wordjspraakverbetering by dowe kinders aangemoedig en look deur hul begeer om onafhanklike funksioneringj in die bree samelewing te vergemaklik (ling, 1976; manilal, 1992). voortgesette pogings tot beter begrip van die spraakproduksie van die dowe en dus meer effektiewe spraakevaluasie en -terapie bly 'n navorsings-prioriteit. afgesien van die swak spraakverstaanbaarheid, word die dowe se spraakpogings ook deur 'n hoe mate van produksie-onstabiliteit of intrapersoonsvariasie in herhaling van dieselfde uitings, gekenmerk (harris, rubin, spitz & mcgarr, 1985; metz, schiavetti, sitler & samar, 1990; osberger, 1987). desnieteenstaande is daar aanduidings dat sprekers konstante verskille tussen sekere uitings aanbring (monsen 1976). hierdie konstante verskille kan as spraakpatrone of spraakreels1 beskou spraakreels is die produksiereels waarvolgens spraakklanke op dieselfde, konstante wyse uitgespreek word en op 'n konstante wyse van ander spraakklanke verskil (verwerk uit grunwell, 1982). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 santie ε meyer monsen, 1976). die verskil in mening is nog nie besleg nie veral aangesien dit vir die spraakopleier2 so problematies is om hierdie spraakreels te identifiseer. die reelmatighede in die produksie van die dowe se spraak is soms deur die normaalhorende luisteraar identifiseerbaar maar in vele gevalle nie. selfs waar die dowe se spraakproduksie deur middel van die internasionle fonetiese alfabet (ifa) getranskribeer word, is die gevaar dat die spreker se komplekse reelsisteem vereenvoudig word deur die transkripsie (parker & rose, 1990). verder is die onakkurate segmente dikwels moeilik artikulatories spesifiseerbaar en dus moeilik verteenwoordigbaar met 'n ifa-simbool (abberton, hazan & fourcin, 1990). dit is dus moontlik dat die dowe reelmatighede in sy eie spraak gebruik wat nie deur die normaalhorende luisteraar identifiseerbaar is nie. die teenwoordigheid en die aard van spraakreels is egter vir die klinikus van groot belang met die oog op die ontwikkeling van rehabilitasiestrategie. terapie is tradisioneel nie dieselfde vir 'n persoon met 'n konstant foutiewe reel, die met variasie in produksie van dieselfde klank en die persoon met geen spraakklankreel nie. die prognose vir terapie sal ook verskillend wees in die geval van 'n individu met geen spraakreel nie en vir die persoon waar daar alreeds 'n foutiewe reel vasgele is (stoel-gammon & dunn, 1985). dit is derhalwe duidelik dat die identifikasie van die dowe se spraakreels, indien enige, belangrik is. tot op hierdie stadium is die probleem dat daar nie 'n klinies bruikbare metode is om die spraakreels te identifiseer nie. die studies wat die reelmatighede aangetoon het, het dit slegs vanuit 'n teoretiese oogpunt benader, instrumentele analises gebruik wat nie vir die klinikus beskikbaar is nie, byvoorbeeld elektromiografiese studies en nie gepoog het om die spraakreels van 'n individu aan te toon nie (byvoorbeeld monsen, 1976; huntington, harris & sholes, 1968). een van die redes is dat slegs enkele klanke ondersoek is en in die meeste gevalle is die studies net op die fonetiese vlak van spraakproduksie, met ander woorde in betekenislose eenhede, uitgevoer (metz, 1980; huntington et al., 1968). die gebruik van die spraakreels as 'n kommunikasiemedium, waar spraakklanke betekenisonderskeidend aangewend word (op die fonologiese vlak), word nie op die wyse ondersoek nie. 'n tradisionele fonologiese ontleding (byvoorbeeld grunwell, 1982) is onbevredigend aangesien die swak spraakverstaanbaarheid van die dowe spreker transkripsie van die spraak feitlik onmoontlik maak. 'n tradisionele "artikulasietoets" het verskeie probleme (ling, 1976) waarvan een is dat die onkonstantheid in die dowe se produksie nie in ag geneem word met die enkele ontlokking van 'n foneem in 'n spesifieke konteks nie. om die spraakreels dus te kan bepaal, moet die metode vir die spraakopleier uitvoerbaar wees en ook in staat wees om die konstanthede, indien teenwoordig in die spraakproduksie van die dowe, uit te lig. die studie het dus ten doel om te bepaal of daar reelmatighede in geselekteerde uitinge van die afrikaanssprekende dowe deur luisteraarsbeoordelings geidenti2 spraakopleier verwys na die persoon wat die spraakopleiding van die dowe doen. dit kan die oudioloog of die onderwyseres indie skool vir dowes wees. fiseer kan word. deur spesifieke spraakvoorbeelde van die dowes op 'n herhaalde wyse te ontlok en luisteraars die voorbeelde te laat beoordeel, kan bepaal word of die dowes foneme konstant produseer en of luisteraars ooreenstem ten opsigte van die foneme wat hulle hoor. metode proefpersone die twee groepe proefpersone, naamlik dowe sprekers en normaalhorende luisteraars word afsonderlik bespreek. dowe sprekers die proefpersone moes verteenwoordigend wees van dowes waar slegs 'n gehoorverlies voorkom het dat die omgewing se spraakreels spontaan aangeleer kon word. hulle moet dus geen ander probleme as slegs 'n ernstige (71-91db) en uitermatige (>91 db) kongenitale sensoriesneurale gehoorverlies openbaar nie en verby die aktiewe periode van spraaken taalaanleer wees (bess & mcconnell, 1981). seuns in standerd agt, nege en tien met ernstige en uitermatige kongenitale sensories-neurale gehoorverliese is in 'n afrikaanse skool vir dowes gei'dentifiseer. die seuns met 'n normale intellektuele vermoe volgens hul skoolprestasie en vorige ik-toetsing en met horende afrikaanssprekende ouers wat sedert kleutertyd 'n skool vir dowes bygewoon het, is vervolgens geselekteer. sewe proefpersone het aan die vereistes voldoen. hulle het 'n gemiddelde spraakverstaanbaarheidskaaltellingvantussen 1.5 en 3.8 op 'n vyf-punt-skaal (meyer, 1984) behaal met een as onverstaanbaar en vyf as volkome verstaanbaar gestel. die skaaltelling is bereken deur die gemiddeld van ses beoordelaars. normaalhorende luisteraars vrywillige studente in hul derde jaar van die ib.log.graad aan die universiteit van pretoria is as beoordelaars van die spraakuitings gebruik. hulle is reeds opgelei om bree transkripsies van abnormale spraak te doen. die studente (ouderdom wissel van 20 tot 22 jaar), beskik oor normale gehoor en het slegs beperkte indien enige blootstelling aan die spraak van dowes gehad. dertien van die studente het afrikaans as moedertaal, terwyl een haarself as tweetalig beskou. 1 apparaat 'n sony stereo cassette corder tc-158 sd bandopnemer met twee sher-o-dyne model 533samikrofone is vir die opname van die spraakvoorbeelde in 'n stil, vertrek by die skool gebruik. die perseptuele beoordelings is in die taallaboratorium van die universiteit van pretoria gedoen. die taallaboratorium is to^gerus met 'n tandberg model is-10 apparaat. elke luisteraar het vir die ouditief perseptuele beoordelings 'n tdk d 60 band ontvang wat deur middel van 'n individuele kopstuk (tandberg tlh 12) en na gelang van persoonlike luidheidsvoorkeur beoordeel moes word, 'n enkele luisteraar het 'n phillips d-6280 bandspeler vir die beoordelings gebruik. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vokaalreels by dowes tabel 1: gehoorgestremde sprekers en eienskappe vir seleksie spreker ouderdom graad van verlies oorsaak van verlies aanvang ouderdom: diagnose ander probleme ik spraak verstaanbaarheid een 17 jr 6 mnde uitermatig onbekend kongenitaal 11 mnde geen 123 1.5 twee 17 jr 3 mnde uitermatig onbekend kongenitaal 17 mnde geen 105 1.8 drie 16 jr 11 mnde ernstig onbekend kongenitaal 30 mnde geen 93 2.7 vier 19 jr uitermatig waardenburg kongenitaal 32 mnde geen 101 2.5 vyf 19 jr 9 mnde uitermatig onbekend kongenitaal 24 mnde geen 122 1.8 ses 18 jr 6 mnde uitermatig onbekend kongenitaal 18 mnde geen 100 3.8 sewe 20 jr 1 mnd uitermatig onbekend kongenitaal 24 mnde geen 114 1.5 materiaal vir die ontlokking van die spraakvoorbeelde in 'n poging om die spraakreels van die dowe op 'n klinies toepasbare wyse te bepaal, is daar verskeie vereistes gestel, naamlik: ο die eksperimentele taak en die materiaal wat gebruik word, moet geskik vir die dowe spreker wees, ο verskeie herhalings van die spraakvoorbeelde moet versamel word sodat reelmatighede of produksiestabiliteit, bepaal kan word, ο die woordstruktuur moes eenvoudig wees sodat dit met gemak deur 'n dowe gelees en uitgespreek kan word. daar is besluit om kvk-woorde te gebruik. die kvkwoorde wat geselekteerj is, is minimale pare om op 'n gekontroleerde analitiese wyse te bepaal of geselekteerde kontraste inderdaad afwesig of teenwoordig is. slegs die toetsvokaal is 'gevarieer en die konsonante is as lb-tl konstant gehou. die konstante klankomgewing is belangrik in die perseptuele beoordelings sodat die beoordelaars nie enige addisionele inligting kry wat die beoordeling van die toetsklank bei'nvloed nie. elke woord is vyf keer herhaal sodat daar van elke vokaal in 'n spesifieke kontekstuele omgewing vyf produksies is. ο dit is nie moontlik om alle spraakreels te bestudeer nie en daarom is slegs vokale geselekteer. vokale is geselekteer na aanleiding van die volgende kriteria: slegs enkelvokale is oorweeg, aangesien die dowe duurverskille in vokaalproduksie, selfs al is die produksie abnormaal, behou en duurverskille in afrikaans kontrasterend gebruik word, byvoorbeeld man teenoor maan (meyer, 1984). die abnormale hoe voorvokaal [y] is ook uitgelaat aangesien die vokaal dikwels as ongerond uitgespreek word en dus as [i] of [θ] realiseer (meyer, 1984). slegs vokale wat in die [b-t] klankomgewing voorkom is verder ingesluit en om die rede is [ae] weggelaat. vokale moes ook verteenwoordigend wees van die afrikaanse vokale en is dus geselekteer om hoe, neutrale en lae, sowel as voor-, middelen agter-vokale in te sluit, naamlik /a, i, u, ε, a, o/ (wissing, 1982; de villiers & ponelis, 1987). die woorde vir die vokaalontleding is: boet, bot, bad, bed, bied en bid. ο aangesien verskeie faktore 'n effek op die duur van klanke het, byvoorbeeld die spraakspoed, die posisie van die woord in 'n sin, die betekenis van die woord, klem van die woord in die sin (nickerson, stevens, boothroyd & rollins, 1974, lehiste, 1970), is alle toetswoorde in dieselfde sin geplaas, naamlik "ek het gese". ο die toetssinne is individueel op wit 10 x 20 cm kaarte aangebring waarop die toetswoord in donker en onderstreepte font verskyn. 'n swart en wit lyntekening wat die woordbetekenis illustreer, is telkens op die kaart aangebring om sodoende die betekenisvolheid van die woord te verseker. optekening van data die beoordelaars het 'n geslotekeuseantwoordblad ontvang waarop hul response aangeteken moet word. die responsmetode is geselekteer aangesien die sprekers as gevolg van hul swak verstaanbaarheid besonder swak sou vaar op 'n onbeperkte-keuse-responswyse (osberger, 1992). die alternatiewe is geselekteer om die korrektheid van die volgende fonetiese kontraste te evalueer, naamlik vertikale tongposisie of tonghoogte (bied/bad; boet/ bad), horisontale tongposisie (bied/boet; bed/bot), lipvorming (bied/ buut; bid/ bot; bed/bot) en duur (bat/ baat; bed/beet; bot/boot). daar was met ander woorde 10 moontlikhede waaruit die luisteraar 'n keuse moes maak sowel as 'n vraagtekenkategorie. die vraagteken-kategorie is ook ingesluit om inligting te verkry wanneer die spreker se distorsie nie 'n bekende identifiseerbare vorm aanneem nie (owens, talbott & schubert, 1968). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 santie meyer prosedure prosedure vir dataversameling 1) 'n stil vertrek is geselekteer sodat die agtergrondgeraas op die opnames so laag moontlik vir die versekering van akkurate analises is. 2) die doel van die eksperiment is kortliks aan elke proefpersoon verduidelik en die eksperimentele taak is uiteengesit. 3) die kaarte met toetsstimuli is omgekeerd op die tafel voor die spreker geplaas en die mikrofone is ongeveer 20 sentimeter regs, voor die spreker se mond op 'n tafel geplaas om die effek van die kamerakoestiek sover moontlik te beperk. 4) daar is dan oorgegaan om die voorbeelde van die toetssinne aan die spreker voor te hou om te verseker dat hy begryp wat van hom verwag word. 5) die sprekers is daarna gevra om die woorde op die kaarte op 'n natuurlike wyse te lees. wanneer die ondersoeker seker was dat die spreker weet wat van hom verwag word en dat hy die opdrag kan uitvoer, is tot die oudio-opnames van die eksperimentele sinne oorgegaan. prosedure vir die perseptuele ontleding 6) 7) 8)' al die beoordelaars is opgelei vir die luistertaak deurdat die navorser twee sprekers se spraak saam met die groep beoordeel het. die beoordelaars het elk hul eie oudioband ontvang waarop die toetssinne van 'n spesifieke spreker gekopieer is met 'n vel papier waarop 'n beperkte keuse van elke toetsitem aangebring is. 'n voorbeeld van die toetsitems is : "ek het bad, baat, boet, bot, boot, biet, beet, bid, bed, buut ? gese." elke spreker se sinne is deur twee luisteraars beoordeel. elke luisteraar is slegs vir die ontleding van een spreker se uitinge gebruik om te voorkom dat daar 'n orde-effek voorkom deurdat die beoordelaars die daaropvolgende sprekers beter beoordeel as gevolg van hul blootstelling aan die navorsingstaak (mcgarr, 1983). analise van data 9) 'n punt is aan elke korrekte foneembeoordeling toegeken. 'n vervangingstelling word ook bereken deur die aard van die beoordelaars se "foutiewe" response (die beoordelings wat nie die mikpuntfoneem bevat nie) te ondersoek. indien dieselfde foneem vir 70 of meer persent waargeneem is, is dit as die vervangingsfoneem beskou. 10) die beoordelaarsresponse is ook nagegaan om 'n kontrastelling op te teken (boothroyd, 1985). die kontraste naamlik tonghoogte of vertikale tongposisie (hoog, middel, laag), horisontale tongposisie (voor, sentraal, agter) , duur (kort, lank) en lipstand (de villiers & ponelis, 1987; wissing, 1982) is ondersoek. die kontraste lewer 'n totaal van 40 (vier kontraste, vyf herhalings, twee luisteraars) moontlike korrekte response per proefpersoon per foneem. 11) die response is verder in die vorm van 'n beoordelingsmatriks vir elke spreker opgestel met die doel om patroonmatigheid in die luisteraarsresponse visueel voor te stel. 12) die interluisteraar-korrelasiekoeffisient is .93 wat dui op betroubare luisteraarsbeoordelings (downie & heath, 1971). resultate resultate van die luisteraarsbeoordelings van die dowes se vokaalproduksie as 'n groep. in tabel 2 word die vokaal wat deur die luisteraars waargeneem is horisontaal en die teikenvokaal vertikaal in die beoordelingsmatriks se selle ingevul. die getal in die selle is die frekwensie waarmee elke teiken-/ waargenome vokaalpaar voorgekom het. die persentasie korrekte produksie sowel as die persentasie waarmee 'n spesifieke vokaal as vervanger gebruik word, word ook verskaf. elke vokaal is deur 14 luisteraars beoordeel. beskrywing van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van die dowes as 'n groep. uit tabel 2 is dit duidelik dat die ses toetsvokale met 'n verskeidenheid vokale vervang word. die sprekers behaal 'n persentasie korrekte produksie van slegs 46.6%!. die tabel 2. beoordelingsmatriks van die gehoorgestremdes se vyf herhalings van die vokale. waargenome foneem a a: i y e: ε β 3 ο: u totaal 1 % korrek 1 a 51 2 0 1 0 7 9 0 0 0 70 72.9' i 14 3 11 1 1 30 9 0 1 0 70 15.7 ε 14 9 4 2 34 7 0 0 0 70 48.6' a 16 1 0 0 0 5 41 0 1 6 70 58.5 d 13 6 2 2 0 1 1 19 1 25 70 x 27.1 u 8 8 1 4 0 0 2 6 1 40 70 57.1 totaal 116 20 23 12 3 77 69 25 4 y 71 420 46.6 % vervang 15.5 4.8 2.9 2.9 .7 10.2 6.7 1.4 .95 7.4 53.4 g ω ω β a α ja f8 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vokaalreels by dowes vokale wat die meeste korrek3 geproduseer word is /a/ (73%), /a/ (59%) en ivj (57%). hierdie foneme, sowel as die hi, word ook algemeen as vervangingsvokale gebruik. dit is verder opvallend dat selfs die beste geproduseerde vokale (/a/ teen 73%) 'n relatief lae persentasie korrekte produksie behaal het. die vokaal wat die meeste probleme opgelewer het, is die /i/ waar die sprekers slegs 16 % behaal. bespreking van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van die dowes as 'n groep. die beoordelingsmatriks (kyk tabel 2) illustreer die swak produksie van vokale duidelik. vokale word deur ten minste drie ander vokale vervang. die swak vokaalproduksie van die dowe spreker is alreeds deeglik in verskeie tale gedokumenteer (angelocci, kopp & holbrook, 1964; meyer, 1984; odendaal, 1981; suonpaa & aaltonen, 1981). die huidige studie bevestig dan ook die resultate waar 'n maksimun persentasie korrekte vokaalproduksie van slegs 47% verkry is. die prestasie is egter beter as in ander studies wat ook op afrikaanssprekende proefpersone uitgevoer is. meyer (1984) verkry byvoorbeeld 33% en odendaal (1981) 21%. die verskil is egter waarskynlik aan die aard van die beoordelingstaak toe te skryf. in hierdie studie is van 'n geslote-keusetaak gebruik gemaak terwyl die ander studies die luisteraars 'n vrye keuse in die beoordelings gegee het. dit is bekend dat die prestasie in 'n geslote-keusetaak beter as in 'n vrye keuse is (osberger, 1992). oor die algemeen blyk dit dat die lae en die sentrale vokale meer dikwels korrek uitgespreek word en dat die voorvokale meer dikwels probleme oplewer (gold, 1980) soos ook in die huidige studie die geval is. die verklaring wat hiervoor voorgehou word, is dat dowes geneig is om hul tonge laag en na agter in die mondholte te hou. daar word gemeen dat dowes die tongstand verkies omdat dit taktiele leidrade verskaf (boone, 1966). die tongplasing het uiteraard 'n negatiewe effek op die voorvokale se resonansiepatrone. j /wanneer die hoorbaarheid van die vokaalformantfrekwensies in gedagtejgehou word, kan die beter produksie van /a/ en ivj verklaar word. beide die vokale se eerste en tweede formante is in 'n relatief lae frekwensiegebied gelee (meyer, 1984). die sentrale /a/ vokaal se relatief geslaagde produksie is ook in ooreenstemming met vorige studies. daar word algemeen gemeen dat die ontspanne natuurlike tongposisie /a/ 'n maklik produseerbare vokaal maak. die relatief ongeslaagde produksie van /a/ (slegs 27% korrekte produksie is behaal) is nie duidelik nie. dit is 'n vokaal waar beide die eerste en die tweede formante in die frekwensiegebied onder 1000 hz gelee is (peterson & barney, 1952) en dus hoorbare leidrade vir die produksie behoort te bied. nogtans berig owens et al. (1968) dat dowes die vokaal dikwels foutief waarneem. daarbenewens is dit ook 'n vokaal wat sigbare leidrade in die vorm van lipronding bied, wat ook korrekte produksie kan bevorder. daar is egter verskeie ander studies wat die dowes se probleme met die vokaal aantoon (meyer, 1984; odendaal, 1981). die swak produksie van ν is egter nie onverwags nie, 3 korrek moet gelees word as "korrek identifiseerbaar" aangesien die spraak van al die sprekers die kenmerkende afwykings van die dowe vertoon. 53 en is ook al in vorige studies en in ander tale bevind (geffner, 1980; meyer, 1984; odendaal, 1981). die hoe voortongposisie verskaf min taktiele leidrade (boone, 1966) en die hoe tweede formantfrekwensie maak die vokaal moeilik hoorbaar (meyer, 1984; peterson & barney, 1952). die spreker het met ander woorde min inligting tot sy beskikking om sy produksie van die vokaal te rig, wat tot die swak produksie aanleiding gee. alhoewel duurversteurings dikwiels in die vokaalproduksie van die dowe se spraak voorkom (parkhurst & levitt, 1978), was dit nie 'n opvallende kenmerk van die huidige studie nie. daar is spesifiek net kort vokale vir die navorsingstaak geselekteer, en die luisteraars het slegs met uitsondering 'n lang /a:/ (4.7%), /e:/ (.7%), en /o:/ (.95%) in plaas van kort vokale gehoor. dit is steeds moontlik dat die sprekers hul toetsvokale verleng, maar dat dit nie as lang vokaal relatief tot die res van die sin gehoor word nie, aangesien die totale uiting se duur verleng is. dit is dan ook reeds opgeteken dat die dowe spreker, ten spyte van algemene verlenging van vokale, die relatiewe duur van sy vokale behou (meyer, 1984). die laaste opvallende bevinding is dat alle vokale nie ewe veel as vervangingsvokale gebruik word nie. die id en die /a/ word algemeen as vervangingsvokale gebruik. die id word hoofsaaklik as die vervangingsvokaal van die ν gebruik. beide vokale is voorvokale en het 'n relatief lae eerste formant. die tweede formant van die id is egter laer as die van die /i/ (meyer, 1984) wat dit waarskynlik makliker hoorbaar maak. die /a/ daarenteen, word vir 'n wye verskeidenheid vokale as vervanging gebruik sodat daar nie so 'n duidelike patroon voorgekom het nie. wat egter opvallend is, is dat /a/ eerder as die neutrale vokaal ibi as "algemene" vervangingsvokaal gebruik word. afgesien van die /a/ se hoorbaarheid soos reeds genoem, bied die middellae tongposisie moontlik taktiele terugvoeringsleidrade wat produksie vergemaklik. die vervangingspatroon het veroorsaak dat die tradisionele vokaalneutraliserings (levitt, stromberg, smith, & gold, 1980) nie in die groep sprekers se spraak opgeval het nie. die bestudering van die data van die groep as geheel, het interessante patrone na vore gebring met die uitstaande indruk dat die dowe spreker oor swak vokaalproduksie beskik. die samevoeging van die groepsdata verskuil egter die moontlikheid van enige hoorbare patroonmatigheid in die vokaalproduksie van die individuele dowe spreker. in 'n poging om die individuele patroonmatigheid na te gaan, sal die beoordelingsmatrikse van die sprekers afsonderlik bestudeer word. resultate van die luisteraarsbeoordelings van individuele dowe sprekers in tabel 3 word die vokaalbeoordelingsmatriks vir spreker een aangetoon. die vokaal wat deur die luisteraars waargeneem is, is horisontaal en die vokaal wat die spreker geproduseer het of die teikenvokaal, vertikaal in die beoordelingsmatriksselle ingevul. die getal in die selle is die frekwensie waarmee elke teiken-/ waargenome vokaalpaar voorgekom het. elke vokaal is deur twee luisteraars beoordeel en die persentasie korrekte beoordelingvir elke vokaal asookdie persentasie korrekte beoordeling vir al die vokale gesamentlik word aangetoon. die asterisk by 'n selinskrywing toon aan dat 'n luisteraar in al vyf beoordelings dieselfde vokaal gehoor het. die persentasie waarmee 'n spesifieke vokaal as vervangingsvokaal gebruik is, word ook aangegee. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 santie ε meyer tabel 3. vokaalbeoordeling van spreker een waargenome foneem tabel 5. vokaalbeoordeling van spreker drie waargenome foneem a v v α £ α v j4 •ρ* β a 1 y e: ε 9 d o: u korrek a 1 y e: ε 9 0 ο: u korrek a 3 6 10 a 10* 100 i 7 3 0 i 1 1 5* 3 10 ε 10* 100 a 0) ε 3 5* 2 50 9 1 9* 90 0) α £ a 10* 100 d 2 8* 20 α 0) j4 •ρ* d 2 8* 20 u 10* 100 0 u 10* 100 % vervang 1.7 0 0 0 16.7 15 0 0 13.3 % vervang 0 0 6.6 0 8.3 8.3 0 0 13.3 % korrekte vokaalproduksie: 53.3% beskrywing van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van spreker een uit tabel 3 blyk dit dat die die spreker 'n persentasie korrekte vokaalproduksie van 53 % behaal het en dat die luisteraars /ε/, h / en /u/ korrek kon hoor. hulle het egter met goeie eenstemmigheid 'n foutiewe maar reelmatige produksie van /i/ as /ε/ en /o/ as /u/ gehoor. die spreker het met ander woorde slegs drie in plaas van die ses vokale in sy foneemskat. die vokale wat hy korrek produseer, word ook as vervangingsvokale vir die ander gebruik. in tabel 4 word die vokaalbeoordelingsmatriks vir spreker twee afsonderlik aangetoon. die uiteensetting is soos in tabel 3. beskrywing van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van spreker twee. uit tabel 4 blyk dit duidelik dat die luisteraars nie enige ander vokaal as /a/ konstant kon hoor nie. die spreker maak met ander woorde nie enige verskil in die produksies van /a/, /i/, /a/ en h i soos deur die luisteraars waargeneem nie. dit is ook interessant dat /ε/ en /u/ vir 'n geringe aantal herhalings as korrek waargeneem is maar dat 'n groot tabel 4. vokaalbeoordelings van spreker twee waargenome foneem a v v α a α . · » α •ρ* β % korrekte vokaalproduksie: 63% aantal van die produksies ook as /a/ gehoor is. in plaas van ses foneme gebruik die spreker slegs een foneem. dit is dus nie onverwags dat hy slegs 23% korrekte vokaalproduksie behaal nie. in tabel 5 word die vokaalbeoordelingsmatriks vir spreker twee afsonderlik aangetoon. die uiteensetting is soos in tabel 3. beskrywing van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van spreker drie. uit tabel 5 blyk dit die luisteraars /a/, /u/ en /a/ konstant korrek kon hoor. die enigste ander vokaal wat 'n konstant hoorbare patroon gegee het, is h i wat as /u/ gehoor is. die ander vokale is as verskillend gehoor, en die spreker behaal 'n relatief hoe persentasie korrekte produksie, naamlik 62%. dit was egter interessant dat die luisteraars nie dieselfde inligting uit die uitings verkiy het nie. die vokaal /ε/ is konstant korrek gehoor deur die een luisteraar en ook as die konstante vervangingsklank vir die ν gehoor (aangedui met *). die ander luisteraar het egter nie tabel 6. vokaalbeoordelings van spreker vier waargenome foneem ' i ' korrekte vokaalproduksie: 27% a 1 y e: ε a d o: u korrek a i y e: ε θ d ο: u korrek a 10* 100 a 7* 3 ΐο i 9* 1 0 i 1 1 5 3 10 ε 6 3 1 30 re ik en fo n ee m ε 1 5* 4 50 9 8* 1 1 0 re ik en fo n ee m 9 10* 100 , d 9* 1 0 re ik en fo n ee m d 2 1 5* y / 2 50 u 6 1 3 30 u 2 2 5* 1 10 % vervang 63.3 0 0 0 0 1.6 0 6.6 1.6 % vervang 0 0 10 0 10 15 8.3 0 3.3 korrekte vokaalproduksie: 48% the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vokaalreels by dowes 55 dieselfde konstante inligting uit die /i/ en id uitings verkry nie, want sy het die vokale, hoofsaaklik lyl of/a/ gehoor. in tabel 6 word die vokaalbeoordelingsmatriks vir spreker twee afsonderlik aangetoon. die uiteensetting is soos in tabel 3. beskry wing van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van spreker vier. uit tabel 6 blyk dit duidelik dat die luisteraars slegs in die geval van /a/ en lal die vokale as korrek kon hoor. die ander vokale is egter nie as patroonmatig waargeneem nie. verskeie vokale is met lal vervang, terwyl id en hi ook dikwels in plaas van ander foneme gehoor is. van die ses toetsvokale wil dit voorkom asof die spreker lal tabel 7. vokaalbeoordelings van spreker vyf waargenome foneem a i y e: ε a d o: u korrek a 8* 2 80 i 5 3 2 0 ε 8 2 20 a 7* 1 2 20 d 4 6 0 u 2 8 0 % vervang 43.3 33.3 0 0 3.3 0 0 0 0 % korrekte vokaalproduksie: 20% tabel 8. vokaalbeoordelings van spreker ses waargenome foneem a i y e: ε a d ο: u korrek a 10* 100 i 10* 100 ε 8 2 20 a 5* 5* 50 d 10* 100 u 1 9* 90 % vervang 0 13.3 0 0 0 0 1.6 0 8.3 % korrekte vokaalproduksie: 76.6% opsigte van die id wat met redelike goeie konstantheid met die lil vervang is nie. daar was egter 'n verskil in opinie tussen die luisteraars oor die korrektheid van die lal. die een het gemeen dat die vokaal korrek uitgespreek is terwyl die ander dit as 'n konstante vervanging met ivj gehoor het. die spreker gebruik vier in plaas van die ses toetsvokale. hy tref geen onderskeiding tussen ν en id nie en sy produksie van lal is nie duidelik nie. hy behaal 62% korrekte vokaalproduksie. in tabel 9 word die vokaalbeoordelingsmatriks vir spreker twee afsonderlik aangetoon. die uiteensetting is soos in tabel 3. kontrasteer met voorvokale wat deur id verteenwoordig word en agtervokale wat deur h / verteenwoordig word. die lal word as algemene vervangingsfoneem aangewend. ten spyte van die beperkte foneemskat behaal die spreker 47% korrekte produksie. j in tabel 7 word die vokaalbeoordelingsmatriks vir spreker twee afsonderlik aangetoon. die uiteensetting is soos in tabel 3. i | beskrywing van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van spreker vyf. uit tabel 7 blyk dit dat die luisteraars net een vokaal, naamlik die /a/, korrek kan identifiseer. die /a/ word dan ook as algemene vervangingsfoneem gehoor. die spreker kontrasteer lal en lul skynbaar deur middel van 'n duurverskil, waar die lul met 'n langer duur uitgespreek word. daar is egter geen ander duidelike patroon wat uit die data na vore kom nie en hy behaal slegs 20% korrekte produksie. in tabel 8 word die vokaalbeoordelingsmatriks vir spreker twee afsonderlik aangetoon. die uiteensetting is soos in tabel 3. beskrywing van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van spreker ses. uit tabel 8 blyk dit dat die luisteraars eenstemmig was ten opsigte van die korrektheid van die produksies van vier van die vokale. daar is ook nie onduidelikheid ten beskrywing van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van spreker sewe. uit tabel 9 blyk dit dat die luisteraars slegs die id en die lul as korrek beoordeel het. hulle was ook eenstemmig dat die spreker nie enige onderskeid tref tussen lil en id nie en ook nie tussen hi en lul nie. die ander beoordelings vertoon nie 'n duidelike patroon nie. hy behaal dan ook slegs 40% korrekte produksie. tabel 9. vokaalbeoordelings van spreker sewe waargenome foneem a i y e: ε a d o: u korrek a 5 1 4 50 i 1 8* 1 0 ε 1 2 7* 70 a 5 5 50 d 2 1 7 0 u 1 2 7* 70 % vervang 0 6.7 5 5 28.3 3.3 0 0 11.7 % korrekte vokaalproduksie: 40% die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 santie ε meyer bespreking van die resultate van die vokaalbeoordelings van individuele dowe sprekers. die individuele beoordelingsmatrikse van die sprekers toon dat daar groot individuele verskille ten opsigte van die persentasie korrekte vokaalproduksie, wat wissel van 20 tot 62 persent, bestaan. alhoewel die luisteraars nie in staat was om al ses vokale in enige van die sprekers se spraakmonsters te identifiseer nie, kon hulle vier by spreker ses teenoor slegs een by spreker twee hoor. nog 'n interessante verskil tussen die verskillende sprekers was die vokale wat die luisteraars as korrek kon identifiseer. die /a:/ is deur 5 sprekers (sprekers twee, drie, vier, vyf en ses) die /u/ deur vier (sprekers twee, drie, vier en ses) en /a/ deur drie (sprekers een, drie, en vier) korrek gebruik. die ander vokale was korrek deur een (/i/, h/) of twee (/ε/) sprekers (sprekers een en sewe) gebruik. dit dui ook net weer op die individuele patrone in die vokaalproduksie by die sprekers. die verskil in die vervangingsvokale wat die luisteraars kon hoor, was ook opvallend tussen die sprekers. die vokale wat as vervangingsvokale gebruik is, was in die meeste gevalle die vokale wat die spesifieke spreker ook korrek kan produseer. dit gee dan outomaties aanleiding tot individuele verskille soos in die vorige paragraaf verduidelik is. alhoewel daar aanvaar word dat dowes nie dieselfde foute vertoon nie, lewer die studie 'n bydrae ten opsigte van die klem wat op individuele foutpatrone geplaas word. groepsdata soos in verskeie studies (gold, 1980; odendaal, 1981) en soos in tabel 2 verskaf, verbloem die individuele foutpatrone wat in die dowes se spraak na vore kom wanneer die individuele data bestudeer word. dit lei tot die foutiewe aannames dat "dowe spraak" nie ook individuele patrone vertoon nie. die individualiteit van 'n spreker se fonologiese stelsel is van groot belang in terapie. oster (1991) noem dat 'n afwykende produksie sistematies en stabiel oor tyd kan wees en dat dit die realisering van 'n afwykende reel kan wees. indien terapie gegee word sonder bewustheid van bestaande kontraste in die kind se spraak en dus bestaande verbindings tussen die fonetiese realisering en die abstrakte linguistiese vlak, kan dit tot die verval van die kind se sisteem en tot 'n afname in verstaanbaarheid lei. dit is verder belangrik om daarvan bewus te wees dat dit nie raadsaam is om op slegs 'n enkele uiting staat te maak om die dowe se produksiekennis te evalueer nie. die variasie in die beoordelings van die sprekers het duidelik uit die beoordelingsmatrikse geblyk. dit is duidelik dat sommige vokale op 'n lukraak wyse geproduseer word, soos blyk uit die /a/ van spreker sewe. die variasie in produksie sou nie uit 'n enkele spraakvoorbeeld duidelik geword het nie. 'n laaste opmerking ten opsigte van die luisteraarsbeoordelings is dat dit wil voorkom asof luisteraars nie dieselfde "perseptuele strategiee" gebruik in die beoordeling van die spraak van die sprekers nie. (wanneer 'n luisteraar telkens in elk van die lyste dieselfde vokaal gehoor het wanneer die spreker 'n spesifieke toetswoord geproduseer het, is dit met 'n asterisk in die matrikse aangetoon.) daar is uiteraard niks vreemd omtrent die beoordelings waar 'n spreker 10 in die selinskrywing ontvang het nie. dit impliseer slegs dat beide luisteraars die geproduseerde vokale as korrek beoordeel het. wanneer een luisteraar egter konstant 'n spesifieke vokaal hoor, terwyl die ander luisteraar konstant 'n ander vokaal hoor (kyk spreker 6, vokaal hi) of verskeie ander vokale hoor (kyk byvoorbeeld spreker 4, vokaal /ε/) kan daar moontlik van verskillende luisterstrategiee gepraat word. om die aspek verder te ondersoek is die interluisteraarsbetroubaarheidskoeffisient van die kontrastellings van die twee luisteraars wat elke spreker beoordeel het, ondersoek. resultate van die interluisteraarsbetroubaarheidskoeffisient van die kontrastellings van die luisteraars in tabel 10 is die interluisteraarsbetroubaarheidskorrelasiekoeffisient vir die twee luisteraars bereken deur die kontrastellings vir die vyf herhalings van elke van die ses toetsvokale teenoor mekaar te stel. beskrywing van die interluisteraarsbetroubaarheidskoeffisient van die kontrastellings van die luisteraars uit tabel 10 blyk dit dat die korrelasiekoeffisiente wissel vanaf 0.49 tot 0.98. die rede waarom daar so 'n goeie korrelasie in sommige en so 'n swak korrelasie in ander gevalle bestaan, kan net oor gespekuleer word, veral gesien in die lig van die interluisteraarkorrelasie van 0.93. luisteraarsbeoordelings is tradisioneel die metode van keuse vir die evaluasie van afwykende spraak (kearns & simmons, 1988). dat dit moeilik vir selfs 'n hoogs opgeleide luisteraar is om die spraak van die dowe te beoordeel, is algemeen bekend (monsen, 1978). dit is problematies om die oorsaak van die fout te onttrek omdat spraak nie 'n eenvoudige liniere string simbole is nie, maar 'n ingewikkelde gekoartikuleerde kode. die feit dat die twee luisteraars egter nie dieselfde kontrastellings behaal nie, dui op verskillende luisterstrategiee. dit is moontlik dat sekere luisteraars van die kontraste wat in die spreker se spraak voorkom, suksesvol kan benut, terwyl dit nie vir 'n ander luisteraar duidelik genoeg is om te benut nie. die abnormaliteit in die spraak van die spreker verbloem moontlik die kontras vir so 'n luisteraar. ! dit is immers bekend dat die luisteraar, selfs 'n opgeleide fonetikus, nie 'n spraakanalise kan doen sonder om deur konvensionele terminologie en linguistiese ondervinding beinvloed te word nie (clark & yallop, |l990). in die geval van hierdie luisteraars, slegs opgelei om bree fonetiese transkripsies te doen, sou bogenoemde invloed tabel 10. korrelasie-koeffisient vir vokaalbeoordelings van elke spreker se twee luisteraars luisteraars van korrelasie-koeffisient spreker 1 .93 spreker 2 •74 spreker 3 .49 spreker 4 . .56 / spreker 5 .98 spreker 6 .76 spreker 7 .71 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vokaalreels by dowes 57 moontlik nog sterker kon wees. die luisteraars wat vir die studie geselekteer is, is waarskynlik tiperend van die persone wat vir die dowe se spraakopleiding verantwoordelik sal wees en die bevindings is dus van belang vir die spraakopleier. in teenstelling met die hipotese van individuele luisterstrategiee, is harris et al. (1985) van mening dat luisteraars, hetsy gesofistikeerd of ongesofistikeerd, nie oor 'n spesiale strategie beskik om dowes se vokale te dekodeer nie. die studie spreek egter nie spesifiek die resultate van individuele luisteraars aan soos die huidige navorsing nie. die feit dat luisteraars in hierdie studie in die beoordeling van dieselfde spreker se uitings verskil, bevestig die hipotese dat daar moontlik van verskillende luisterstrategiee sprake is. wanneer die uitings waaroor hulle saamstem bestudeer word, kom interessante inligting na vore en dit word vervolgens verskaf. resultate van die ooreenstemming in die luisteraarsbeoordelings van die dowe sprekers se segmentele produksie in tabel 11 word die resultate van die twee luisteraars se beoordelings van elke spreker weergegee. wanneer sewe van die tien beoordelings ooreengestem het, word die resultaat as 'n duidelike patroon in die produksie van die uiting beskou. wanneer al die beoordelings egter tussen slegs twee segmente gewissel het met ongeveer ewe veel beoordelings in elk is dit ook aangetoon soos byvoorbeeld in die geval van die a/u-inskrywings. indien die luisteraars se beoordelings oor 'n aantal selle verspreid was, is daar nie 'n hoorbare konstantheid in die produksie van die spesifieke foneem nie. beskrywing en verklaring van die ooreen-stemming in die luisteraarsbeoordelings van die dowe sprekers se segmentele produksie uit tabel 11 blyk dit duidelik dat daar sekere foneme is wat meer dikwels deur die luisteraars korrek waargeneem kon word as ander. die /a/ is 'n mikpuntfoneem wat deur vyf of ses van die sewe sprekers gebruik is. die vokale /i/ en hi lewer opvallend dikwels probleme. die luisteraars kon selde hoor dat 'n spreker die mikpuntfoneme konstant korrek gebruik. j dit val verder op dat die sprekers nie een dieselfde vervangingspatroon vertooh nie. alhoewel daar ooreenkomste tussen die sprekers is, is daar nie by enige twee sprekers presies dieselfde mikpuntfoneme wat korrek gebruik word of wat met dieselfde foneem vervang word nie. dit is ook duidelik uit tabel 11 dat die sprekers sommige van die kontraste van die korrekte mikpuntfoneem behou het, byvoorbeeld in die geval van /i/ is die vervangingsfoneem /ε/. beide die teikenen die vervangingsvokaal is voorvokale alhoewel die plek van vonning nie korrek is nie. die individualiteit van die sprekers se segmentproduksie blyk duidelik uit die bespreking. die sprekers het patroonmatigheid getoon en mikpuntfoneme op 'n konstante wyse gerealiseer sodat beide beoordelaars oor ten minste 70% van dieselfde foneem se realiserings saamgestem het. dit dui op die gebruik van kontraste op 'n sistematiese wyse. die individualiteit van die sprekers se foutpatrone is waarskynlik te wyte aan hul individuele sensoriese en omgewingsomstandighede. gevolgtrekking die resultate van die studie toon aan dat dit wel moontlik is om spraakreels in geselekteerde uitinge van die afrikaanssprekende dowe deur middel van luisteraarsbeoordelings te bepaal. uit hierdie studie het daar belangrike feite na vore gekom. eerstens het dit geblyk dat die luisteraars se segmentbeoordelings aangetoon het dat die groep dowe sprekers se produksie in 'n groot mate ooreenstem met wat reeds in die literatuur berig is. die lae voorkoms van korrek gei'dentifiseerde foneme is reeds in verskeie studies aangetoon en is weer in die studie bevestig. tweedens was dit duidelik dat sekere mikpuntfoneme met konstantheid gerealiseer word terwyl dit nie by ander die geval is nie. dit was ook opvallend dat die dowes individuele verskille getoon het ten opsigte van die vokale wat hulle korrek kon produseer en die vokale wat hulle as vervangingsvokale gebruik. derdens is daar bevind dat daar verskille tussen luisteraars na vore getree het. die verskille toon aan dat die luisteraars, indien hulle dieselfde produksie moet beoordeel, nie noodwendig dieselfde foneem identifiseer nie. die metode wat hier gebruik is, het beperkinge aangesien die monsters nie spontane spraak is nie en die linguistiese vaardigheid van die spreker nie bepaal word nie. die verstaanbaarheid van die uitings sal bei'nvloed word deur die sintaktiese en leksikale probleme wat nie op die wyse gei'dentifiseer word nie (boothroyd, 1985). deur egter die uitings te beperk, te laat herhaal en op 'n toetsfoneem spreker 1 spreker 2 spreker 3 spreker 4 spreker 5 spreker 6 spreker 7 aantal korrek a geen a a a a a geen 5 i ε a geen geen a/a: i ε 1 ε ε geen geen ε a i ε 3 a a a θ a a a/u a/ε 3 3 u i a u geen a/a: 3 u 1 u u geen u geen a: u u 4 tabel 11. konstanthede identifiseerbaar in elke gehoorgestremde spreker se vokaalproduksie deur twee luisteraars. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 santie meyer geslotekeusebasis te beoordeel, kan die spraakopleier 'n idee vorm of'n segment op 'n konstante wyse uitgespreek word en die spreker dus oor 'n reel vir die produksie beskik. die kennis kan tot meer effektiewe terapiebeplanning bydra. erkennings die personeel en die skoliere van die tvansoranje-skool vir dowes word vir hul vriendelike samewerking bedank. erkenning word gegee aan die finansiele bystand van die raad van geesteswetenskaplike navorsing. opvattings en gevolgtrekkings wat gemaak word, is die van die skrywer en word nie noodwendig deur die raad onderskryf nie. verwysings abberton, e. hazan, v. & fourcin, a. (1990). the development of contrastiveness in profoundly deaf children's speech. clinical linguistics and phonetics, 4, 209-220. angelocci, α., kopp, g. & holbrook, a. (1964). the vowel formants of deaf and normal hearing eleven to fourteen year old boys. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 29, 156-170. bess, f.h. & mcconnell, f.e. (1981). audiology education, and the hearing impaired child. st. louis: the c.v. mosby company. boone, d.r. (1966). modification of the voices of deaf children. the volta review, 68, 686-692. boothroyd, a. (1978). speech perception and sensori neural hearing loss. in ross, m. & giolas, t.g. (eds). auditory management of hearing impaired children. principles and prerequisites for intervention. baltimore: university park press. boothroyd, a. (1985). evaluation of speech production of the hearing impaired: some benefits of forced-choice testing. journal of speech and hearing research, 28, 185-196. clark, j. & yallop, c. (1990). an introduction to phonetics and phonologyoxford: basil blackwell. de villiers, m. & ponelis, f.a. (1987). afrikaanse klankleer. kaapstad: tafelberg-uitgewers. downie, n.m.& heath, r.w. (1970). basic statistical methods (3rd ed.) new york: harper & row publishers. fry, b.d. (1978). the role and primacy of the auditory channel in speech and language development. in: ross, m. and giolas, t.g. (eds). auditory management of hearing impaired children. principles and prerequisites for intervention. baltimore: university park press. geffner, d. (1980). feature characteristics of spontaneous speech production in young deaf children. journal of communication disorders, 13, 443-454. gold, t. (1980). speech production in hearing-impaired children. journal of communication disorders, 13, 397-418. grunwell, p. (1982). clinical phonology. london: croomhelm. harris, k.s., rubin-spitz, j. & mcgarr, n.s. (1985). the role of production variability in normal and deviant developing speech. proceedings of the conference on the planning and production of speech in normal and hearing impaired individuals. asha reports, 15, 50-57. huntington, d.a., harris, k.s. & sholes, g.n. (1968). an electromyographic study of consonant articulation in hearing impaired and normal speakers. journal of speech and hearing research, 11, 147-158. .reams, k.p. & simmons, n.n. (1988). interobserver reliability and perceptual ratings: more than meets the ear. journal of speech and hearing research, 31, 131-136. lehiste, i. (1970). suprasegmentals. cambridge: the mit press. levitt, h., stromberg, h., smith, c. & gold, t. (1980). the structure of segmental errors in the speech of deaf children. journal of communication disordersf 13, 419-441. ling, d. (1976). speech and the hearing impaired child: theory and practice. washington, d.c.: alexander graham bell association for the deaf. manilal, s. (1992). attitudes of deaf adolescents towards speech language hearing therapy. unpublished ba (sp & th) research report, department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. mcgarr, n. (1983). the intelligibility of deaf speech to experienced and inexperienced listeners. journal of speech and hearing research, 26, 451-458. metz, d.e. (1980). morphological boundaries and the timing of stressed vowels produced by profoundly hearing impaired adults. journal of phonetics, 8, 63-68. metz, d.e., schiavetti, n., sitler, r.w. & samar, v.j. (1990). speech production stability characteristics of hearing impaired speakers. the volta review, 92, 223-235. meyer, s.e. (1984). die vokaalproduksie van die afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremde. ongepubliseerde m.log verhandeling, universiteit van pretoria. monsen, r.b. (1974). durational aspects of vowel production in the speech of deaf children. journal of speech and hearing research, 11, 386-398. monsen, r.b. (1976). the production of english stop consonants in the speech of deaf children. journal of phonetics, 4, 29-41. monsen, r.b. (1978). toward measuring how well hearing impaired children speak. journal of speech and hearing research, 21, 197-219. nickerson, r., stevens, k., boothroyd, a. & rollins, a. (1974). some observations on timing in the speech of deaf and hearing speakers. report no 2905. cambridge, ma:bolt beranek and newman, inc. odendaal, j.e. (1981). 'n ondersoek na luisteraars se beoordeling van die artikulasie van vokale deur dowe en normaalhorende sprekers. ongepubliseerde ba (log) verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria. osberger, m.j. (1987). training effects on vowel production by two profoundly hearing impaired speakers. journal of speech and hearing research, 30, 24-251. osberger, m.j. (1992). speech intelligibility in the hearing impaired: research and clinical applications. in: kent, r.d. (ed). intelligibility in speech disorders. theory, measurement and management. amsterdam: john benjamins publishing co. oster, a-m. (1991). phonological assessment of eleven prelingually deaf children's consonant production. speech transmission laboratoryquarterly progress and status report, 2-3, 11-18. j owens, e., talbott, c.b. & schubert, e.d. (1968). vowel discrimination of hearing-impaired listeners. journal of speech and hearing research, 11, 648-655. | parker, a. & rose, h. (1990). deaf children's phonological development. in: grunwell, p. (ed). developmental speech disorders. edinburgh: churchill livingstone. i parkhurst, b. & levitt, h. (1978). the effect of selected prosodic errors on the intelligibility of deaf speech. journal of communication disorders, 11, 249-256. peterson, g. & barney, h. (1952). control methods used in a study of the vowels. journal of the acoustical society of america, 24, 175-184. stoel-gammon, c. & dunn, c. (1985). normal and disordered phonology in children. baltimore; md: university park press. suonpaa, j. & aaltonen, o. (1981). intelligibility of vowels in words uttered by profoundly hearing-impaired children. journal of phonetics, 9, 445-450. wissing, d.p. (1982). algemene en afrikaanse/generatiewe fonologie. johannesburg: macmillan. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 17 the development of a screening schedule for use by teachers to describe the communication abilities of children with severe disabilities anna-marie wium and erna alant centre for augmentative and alternative communication department communication pathology university of pretoria abstract the study developed a screening schedule for teachers to describe the communication abilities of children with severe disabilities teachers were trained in the use of such an assessment procedure. both the schedule as well as the training were evaluated. results indicated that ike schedule can be regarded as adequate for future use as teachers found it easy to use and applicable to their situation. scoring the schedule requires more practise and training as this is a more advanced skill. specific issues had an effect on the results, e.g., the teachers'familiarity with the type of disability observed, their qualifications and experience. opsomming 'n siftingsskedule vir onderwyseresse is ontwikkel wat die kommunikasievaardighede van kinders met erge gestremdhede beskryf. die onderwyseresse moes opleiding ontvang om die skedule te gebruik. daar is gevind dat die skedule geskik is vir toekomstige gebruik aangesien dit maklik is om te gebruik en toepaslik is vir die klaskamer. gradenng word beskou as η gevorderde vaardigheid wat meer ervaring en opleiding verg. aspekte wat die resultate beinvloed het, was die onderwysers se bekendheid met die tipe gestremdheid, hulle kwalifikasies en tot 'n mindere mate hulle ondervindmg met erg gestremde kinders. key words: screening schedule, teachers, children with severe disabilities, augmentative and alternative communication. introduction the increasing need for service delivery to all children with severe disabilities (csd) necessitates the use of teachers to facilitate the children's general interaction. teachers are ideally suited to assess their childrens' capabilities as they are familiar with their individual needs and skills. in this context, the transdisciplinary method of intervention whereby the teacher is trained to fulfil certain intervention functions in the classroom can be a most effective way of dealing with the'severely disabled population (hogg & raynes, 1978). as assessment can be seen as the beginning of the intervention process, teachers should also be trained to perform this task. researchers such as skuy, westaway, makula and perold (1988) have found teachers valuable, parsimonious and accurate in rating students' performances in psychometric testing. the use of teachers in the communication assessment of csd becomes especially relevant as they are able to functionally assess students during daily class routines. a functional assessment can thus be described as an assessment method aimed at describing the child's performance in real life contexts. this approach to assessment is of the utmost importance when working with the majority of children with severe disabilities as they experience difficulty in learning new tasks due to the severity of their cognitive and/or physical disabilities. it is therefore important that the tasks used in the assessment process should be relevant and applicable to daily routines to facilitate the children's increasing independence. which evaluative procedure should be used if teachers are involved in the assessment process? a very limited choice of non-verbal tests are available, e.g., the nonspeech test (huer, 1983), and are for exclusive use by speech and language therapists. many speech and language therapists who work with csd make use of tests which were developed to test language in depth. the problem is that many of the children with severe disabilities are unable to speak which makes it very difficult to obtain reliable results. the need for an aac assessment schedule to describe the skill areas of functioning relevant to aac which could be used by teachers of csd is evident. however, various issues must be taken into account in the development of such an assessment schedule. firstly, the level of impairment in csd often necessitates the use of an augmentative system for more effective communication. the variation in symptoms of the child with severe disabilities clearly indicates that more than one skill area can be involved. it is therefore important to assess all skill areas involved in augmentative communication, such as communication, cognitive, motor, sensory and social/emotional skills. successful intervention should not take only abilities into account, but should also focus die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 on needs (beukelman & mirenda, 1992) secondly the heterogeneity of this population means that every^ child has unique problems, abilities and needs. a schedule has to address the development of an individual programme. thirdly, the characteristics of the child with severe disabilities such as his inconsistent behaviour, passivity, short attention span, anti-social behaviour (durant, 1990) and motor problems (bergen, 1990), make it necessary to observe such behaviour during ordinary functional routines as was recently proposed by asha (1992). researchers such as light (1989) as well as mirenda and iocono (1990), regard functional communication as an integral part of all areas of development particularly in the areas of social, emotional and cognitive development which must therefore also be accommodated in the assessment process of csd. fourthly, for a schedule to be effective for use in the classroom, it needs to be easy to administer and time effective. the development of such a screening schedule for teachers of csd has various advantages in that its use would enable teachers to describe their students' problems, it would help them to know when to refer to specialised services if necessary, and lastly, they would be able to draw up an individualised program plan for each child which enables them to monitor progress. it is against this background that the present study was conducted to investigate the use of an aac screening schedule for use by teachers. method aims anna-marie wium and erna alant apart from their subjective evaluation on the ease of use of the schedule, the results of their assessment were compared to those done by a group of three experienced aac specialists to assess their ability to identify items and to score them. the results obtained by this group was, considered as a norm or a reference to guide the training of observation skills. research design a small group, experimental design was used, which involved elements of the single subject, multiple-baseline design (using replication across 12 subjects). this is similar to the study described by light, dattilo, english, gutierrez and harts (1992). in this referred study of light et. al (1992), data was presented for each individual subject. data for the evaluation of changes that had occurred in the teachers as a result of the training in observation skills were collected five times in total for each of the 12 teachers and then compared to a norm. this was done for reliability as it is recommended by barlow and hersen (1984) that at least four baselines be collected for convincing results in this type of design. the general flow of events in the design of this study encompassed the following: firstly the development of the schedule which, in turn, was evaluated for ease of use in a pilot study with a sample of eight teachers. after the schedule had been refined, the main study trained twelve teachers to use it. the results were processed and analyzed. to describe the application of a schedule to identify the communication abilities of csd as used by teachers of children with severe disabilities (csd). more specifically the following aspects of implementation will be highlighted: the evaluation of the ease of use of the schedule the identification of questions in the schedule which were poorly phrased and thus poorly understood by the teachers the identification of problematic questions which were difficult to score teachers used the schedule to functionally assess the abilities of three children with severe disabilities (cases a, β and c) and were then asked to evaluate the schedule. materials used the schedule which was used to document observation skills of the teachers was designed to cover 47 items of five skill areas associated to aac. they are the communication, cognitive, motor, sensory and social/emotional skill areas. the questions were formulated in user-friendly terms and examples of these can be seen in tables 1 and 3. the schedule has to be completed by observing four functional tasks. each task needs to be rated on a six-point rating scale (0-5). in order to guide the teacher, the rating scale is categorized as poor, sub-average and average which has to be selected prior to attributing a specific score. table 1 has a summarized description of the schedule1. table 1: description of schedule description of schedule rating scale schedule had to be completed by observing and scoring four functional tasks. five skill areas of functioning related to aac were included and were desribed by 47 questions (items): 1. communication skills: (e.g. "to what extent does he want to communicate with others?") 2. cognitive skills: (e.g. "how well does he search for a hidden object?") 3. motor skills: (e.g. "how well can he reach with his right arm?") 4. sensory skills: (e.g. " how well does the child see for communication purposes?") 5. social/emotional skills: (e.g. " how well does he make eye contact during communication?") six-point rating scale was guided by three categories: poor: 0 1 sub-average: 2 3 average: 4 5 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h p d e v e l o p m e n t of a screening schedule for use by abilities of children with severe disabilities data collection procedure training was conducted as a workshop according to nciples r e c o m m e n d e d for adult training (wium, 1994). tvaining took place at the centre for augmentative and alternative communication (caac), dept. communicaf r n pathology, university of pretoria, over a two-day period the content and procedure of training are described table 2. data collection procedures are highlighted. tfcble 2: training procedure and data collection teachers to describe the communication data analysis procedures 19 the focus of this study was to.develop an aac assessment schedule which involved two aspects: firstly, the schedule had to be evaluated and secondly, the ability of the teachers to identify (recognize) the various items and to score them in the schedule had to be described. evaluation of the schedule by the teachers: the teachers evaluated the ease of use of the schedule by cornday 1:descriptive information of teachers: teachers from schools for children with severe disabilities in the pretoria area registered for the workshop. each received a folder consisting of handouts, three unused schedules and each of the three above-mentioned questionnaires. these teachers had to have a minimum experience of six months with children with severe disabilities. the seats were arranged in a semi-circle facing the researcher/trainer in order to encourage interaction amongst the trainees. each seat was numbered according to their anti-clockwise position from 1-12 (from right to left) for material to be distributed and collected. pre-training knowledge of skill areas: prior to training, teachers completed an open-ended questionnaire which recorded their untrained knowledge of the five skill areas to be assessed in aac as well as their expectations of the course. these questionnaires were collected after completion. introductory and background information on a functional assessment was presented by means of audio-visual material (overhead projector and transparencies). the schedule (see table 1) was introduced and the questions as well as the instructions for scoring were read through with them and demonstrated. teachers were required to score every item in every observation. the assessment schedules were not collected until the teachers had scored four children and had calculated the results. pre-training observation skills (al): firstly they were shown a video (a) of an autistic child (noted as observation al). after three consecutive viewin'gs, they were requested to complete the schedule. the use of the schedule specifies that four functional tasks should be observed and documented. this first documentation was regarded as task 1 on the schedule. after completion of the first observation, questions were answered and the scoring of some of the items was compared within the group as part of the discussion regarding their problems. pre-training observation skills ( b l ) : they were then shown a second video (b) of a cei-ebral palsied child (noted as observation bl) three times consecutively and they were required to complete the schedule as for task 2 on the schedule (see appendix a). the same procedure as in observation a1 was followed after training. again teachers required that scores of some items be compared. these were then discussed in the class. training in the completion of the schedule was conducted by means of video material of children with severe disabilities. atraining video of a young, non-ambulatory, cognitively high-functioning child was shown (as many times as needed) and scored by all teachers. the child was severely communicatively disabled but demonstrated all precursors to communication while playing with a speech-language therapist during therapeutic prone standing. open discussion of all items in the schedule was continuously encouraged. teachers scored this child as the third of the four tasks on their schedules. scoring of these items were compared and discussed by the group. practical demonstrations and role play were used to demonstrate the items in the schedule. day 2: at the beginning of the second day handouts for the specific day were distributed. the previous day's work was reviewed, questions answered and handouts were discussed. another training video of a non-verbal child was then shown as many times as necessary. the child identified pictures and symbols and the teachers had to score this video as well. the training video was discussed and scores compared. teachers have now completed the four tasks on the schedule and could then learn to calculate the results. each teacher had to calculate her four observations (al, bl, task 3 and task 4), and present the results as a graph. the completed schedules were then collected. documentation of tasks 3 and 4 were not used for the research but merely to train teachers in the use of the schedule, the various skill areas and to calculate the results. post-training observation skills (a2): after tea, the first video (a) was again shown (as often as necessary) and scored on an unused schedule as observation a2. questions were again answered and several scores compared. post-training observation skills (b2): the second video (b) was then shown (as often as necessary) and then scored as observation b2. questions were again answered and several scores compared. post-training observation skills (c): a third video (of an unfamiliar child) was then shown and also scored. this observation was documented as observation c. results were compared and questions anwered. the second schedule (with the results of documentation after training), were then collected. post-training evaluation of schedule: teachers had to complete a closed-ended questionnaire to evaluate the schedule. post-training knowledge of skill areas and evaluation of the training: they also had to complete the open-ended questionnaire after training to assess the change that had occurred in their knowledge of the skill areas. comments and recommendations regarding the training were also given. at the end of day 2 teachers received certificates of attendance. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 pleting a closed-ended questionnaire. responses were calculated as percentages and displayed as a distribution graph. comments and recommendations were tabled. evaluation of the items in the schedule: the approach followed emphasised the different interpretations given to the same set of data, and focused on the ability to observe the items listed in the schedule from the observation of video cases as well as an evaluation of their ability to score the items. the ability to identify items described in the schedule was evaluated by using the data obtained from the schedules completed during training. all correct responses were given a value of 1 and all errors a value of 0 which implied that they could or could not identify the skill. responses were considered as correct when the teachers' responses were the same as the norm (which was established by three aac experts prior to the main study). a distribution of all 0's and l's was calculated before (al and b l ) and after (a2, b2, c) training for all teachers and presented in two graphs. four types of results were observed. they are summarised in table 3. the results of both observations a and b, before (al, bl) and after (a2,b2) training, were calculated as percentages and visually presented in figures 3 and 4. questions in the schedule which caused confusion ( 1 0 ) , or where no gains were made (0 0), should be altered to be better understood or should be specifically focused upon in training. on the other hand, questions where existing knowledge (1 1), or gained knowledge (0 1) were observed, could be regarded as adequate for future use. evaluation of scoring this information was obtained by assessing the margin of error exhibited for each question. therefore all scores from the completed schedules were compared to a norm (the value of which was allocated by a specialist team prior to training), and then statistically analysed.. this analysis was done by obtaining the root mean square (rms) (kirkpatrick, 1974) of the deviations from the norm for all 5 observations (al, bl, a2, b2, c), and then comparing them. this information in turn determined not only the magnitude of errors per question, but also the degree of complexity of each case. the deviations from the norm were calculated by subtracting the teacher's score from the norm. the rms's of these deviations were then used as basis of assessing the questions: the larger the deviation from the norm, the less accurate the observation. an overall impression of suitable questions can be obtained by anna-marie wium and erna alant considering the distribution of the deviations. this is presented in fig. 7, where the number of cases (expressed as a percentage of the total number), was grouped in intervals of 25% of the deviation from the norm. the accuracy of teachers' observations as they deviated from the norm was calculated for the individual questions for all three cases (a,b,c). the scores documented before training were compared with those after training and indicated questions to be changed. in calculating an average error per question for all cases (a,b,c) observed, an indication of problematic questions could be obtained. questions with a >1.5 variation were considered as those questions which elicited poor scoring accuracy and should therefore be changed or receive more attention in future training. questions with a <1.5 accuracy were considered adequate for future use. results and discussion the schedule was evaluated from two perspectives: the teachers evaluated it for ease of use content and structure of the schedule were evaluated by identifying questions (items), that proved to be difficult to assess. evaluation of the schedule by teachers the data from this evaluation was obtained from a closed-ended questionnaire and the results are presented as a distribution graph. the results describing the ease of use of the schedule are presented in figure 1 below. from figure 1 it is clear that in general the teachers regarded the schedule as good or very good, and easy to use. none of the teachers evaluated any aspect on this questionnaire as poor or unacceptable although they had these options to choose from if they wanted to. the instructions for use as well as the questions within the schedule were regarded as clear, and its appearance was well received. the length of the schedule for classroom use was considered acceptable (the alternative options to choose from were "too long" or "too short") which actually reflects the optimum choice. in general, the schedule was evaluated very favourably by the teachers and they thought that it would be of great future use to them. j evaluation of content versus structure of the sched ule the problematic questions in the schedule were identified by using a two-level approach: ] table 3: four types of results for identification of items (0-1): gains made by training in ability to identify a function. the number of teachers who could not identify the function before training but could do so afterwards. a decrease of errors after training made by the group indicates that they had gained in observation skills regarding their ability to identify functions. (0-0): no gains made. this implies that the teacher could not identify the function before training and neither could she do it after training. ίβ^ββλη^ϋίβλ existing knowledge. these results imply that the teacher could identify the function before as well as after training. e^bp^blie^pllll^llillii incorrect the teacher identified the correct function before training but could not do so after training. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a screening schedule for use by abilities of children with severe disabilities firstly, it was established whether teachers could recognise the particular skill (described by the item in the schedule) as exhibited by the student thus whether the skill was displayed. secondly the accuracy with which teachers could score (rank) their observation on a six-point rating scale was investigated. it was necessary to differentiate between these two aspects in assessing a particular item to prevent confusion. it is much less difficult to identify an item (skill) as present or absent by attributing a positive (1) or negative (0) value than to scale (score) it from 0 5. scoring could be considered a more advanced skill which requires extensive training. the data (collected before and after training), was used to measure the success with which teachers could recognise items in the schedule as well as to identify questions where items were unreliably scored on a rating scale. recognition of items for all items and for individual cases the questions of concern are those where the item could not be recognised correctly after training. a summary of correct and incorrect answers (after training) was comteachers to describe the communication 21 piled, and is presented in figure 2. from this figure it is clear that after training approximately 31% of the items were identified incorrectly, while 69% were correctly recognised as being present or absent. it is significant to note that the fact that they could identify 69% of the questions before training, could imply that the questions in the schedule were formulated in a user friendly manner and that most people could understand it without any training. an analysis of the schedule for cases a and β together may be misleading, therefore cases a and β were studied separately. these results are presented in figures 3 and 4, where the results obtained prior to training were also included. four classes of answers could therefore be distinguished and are presented in table 3. general analysis of individual cases the sum of the first two categories in the correct group (1:1, 0:1), corresponds with the "correct" class in figure 2, while the latter two can be regarded as being equivalent to the "incorrect" class in that figure. from figure 3 it is apparent that for case a, a total of 65.7% (45.7% + 20.0%) is correct after training, compared with 69.3% for both cases. s c h e d u l e ease of feraphing ease of arithmetic ease of scoring ease of assessment tv clear understood questions understood instructions length appearance c o m p a r i s o n of o b s e r v a t i o n s e v a l u a t i o n o f q u e s t i o n s conect 69% inconect 31% figure 1: teachers' evaluation of the schedule c o m p a r i s o n o f o b s e r v a t i o n s p r e & p o s t t r a i n i n g : c a s e a incorrect 22.5% correct 45.7% correct/incorrect 11.7% incorrect/correct 20.0% figure 2. total of all questions c o m p a r i s o n o f o b s e r v a t i o n s p r e & p o s t t r a i n i n g : c a s e β incorrect 15.4% correct 57.3% correct/incorrect 11.7% incorrect/correct 15.6% figure 3. difficult questions in observation a figure 4. difficult questions in observation β die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 in figure 4, the total of correct items in case β amounts to 72.9% (57.3% + 15.6%), or a difference of 7,2% between the two cases. . it is also important to evaluate the impact of the training: for case a (figure 3), the correctness of answers improved for 20% of the answers, and decreased for 11.7% of the answers (a net gain of 8.3%). for case b, (figure 4), a net gain of 3.9% was recorded which is much less than for case a. (a) questions litems in the schedule that the teachers found difficult to recognise the results were finally used to identify individual questions where teachers found it difficult to recognise the presence or absence of a skill. these results are presented in figures 5 and 6 and should be looked at as a pair in order to distinguish different patterns in shading. particular attention should be paid to the darker and solid areas as these represent correctly identified items, while the lighter areas represent incorrect observations. it is obvious when observing figures 5 and 6, that there is a marked difference in the patterns between cases a and b. questions which were poorly identified in case a were much better identified in case b, and vice versa. a good example of this is where question no. 7 was poorly identified by 9 (75%) of the teachers in case a but only by 2 (16%) in case β after training. this shows that the teachers understood the questions but that they might have had other difficulties, e.g., unfamiliarity with the case presented (which is discussed later). by studying figures 5 and 6 specific questions were identified as difficult. all the questions which could not be identified by more than 25% of the teachers after training, were regarded as difficult and are listed in table 4 below. it was only after all these questions were listed (see table 4), that similarities could be detected which in turn identified problematic questions. questions which were repeatedly regarded as difficult in both cases a and b, consist of 12% (6 out of 47) of the total number of questions. these questions (all ab or ba responses), were labelled as problematic and are numbers 21, 26, 31, 33, 38 and 39. these questions need more attention during training by more demonstrations and explanations. the motor skill area seems to be relatively difficult as 2/3 of the questions in this skill area are listed as difficult, which also includes half of the problematic questions on this list. this indicates that teachers found the terminology and assessment of anna-marie wium and erna alant c o m p a r i s o n o f o b s e r v a t i o n s p r e a n d p o s t t r a i n i n g : c a s e a 12 10 c 8 ο s 1 6 h. h) ο 4 2 11 ι τ υ y c o m m u n i c a t i o n • incorrect [g cor/incorrect ρ ί cognitive m o t o r s e n s o r y s o c i a l ] incor/correct gg correct figure 5. difficult items to identify in observation a c o m p a r i s o n o f o b s e r v a t i o n s p r e a n d p o s t t r a i n i n g : c a s e β c o m m u n i c a t i o n ^cognitiv^^ m o t o r >4 s e n s o r y ̂ "^socfef • incorrect m cor/incorrect m incor/correct g c o r r e c t figure 6. difficult items to identify in observation β a c c u r a c y in s c o r i n g s u m m a r y o f d i s t r i b u t i o n o f e r r o r s 10% 15% 20% p e r c e n t a g e of q u e s t i o n s 25% figure 7. categorised results of the distribution of errors the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a screening schedule for use by abilities of children with severe disabilities motor skill area difficult. the majority of teachers in this study had not yet worked with physically disabled hildren and therefore had no knowledge of this skill area. this is therefore not a reflection on the schedule, but more on the teachers' experience. (b) teachers' accuracy in scoring (rating 0-5) the items in the the schedule the data obtained from the completed schedules also provided information on the questions which elicited poor scoring. teachers scored their observations which were compared with the norm, on a scale from 0 to 5. the size of the deviation from the norm indicated their ability/inability to score a specific item, e.g., when the teachers' scores deviated more than two numerals on a scale from 0-5 then their scoring ability was considered as poor. when their scores deviated one numeral (on a scale 0-5), then teachers to describe the communication 23 their scoring ability was considered as inconsistent. an overall impression of suitable questions can be obtained by considering the distribution of the deviations. this, is presented in figure 7, where the number of cases (expressed as a percentage of the total number), was grouped in intervals of 25% of the deviation from the norm. from figure 7 it is clear (when adding the number of questions in the two intervals of deviations between 26% and 75% from the norm), that there is a large percentage (45%) of questions in this section. also, none of the questions deviates more than 200% (2 numerals on the scale 0-5), from the norm. it must be made clear that the term % might be misleading and should be regarded as a unit (e.g., 100% deviates in fact 1 score from the norm). a more detailed account of the specific problematic questions was obtained when the average error per question was calculated and it became clear that 12,7% of the questions deviated more than 150%. the specific questions are table 4: difficult questions to identify no question prior tu training after training co mmunication skills 1 how well does he respond to his name being called? a 3 to what extent does he look at preferred item to indicate choice? a 7 how well does he make sounds? a a. 11 how well does he use questions? β β 12 how well does he use yes/no responses? β β 15 how well is his speech understood by others? β β co gnitive skills 18 how well can he imitate movements after a model? a a 21 how well does he follo.w a one-step command/request ? ab ab 23 how well does he use jan object for what it is meant for? a a mc >tor skills 26 how well is the student positioned for function? β ba 28 how good is his muscle tone? (floppy/very stif!/mixed=0, normal=5)? β β 30 how well can he reach with right arm (range of motion)? β β 31 how well can he reach with left arm (range of motion)? ab ab 33 how good is the fine co-ordination in his dominant/user hand? ab ab 34 to what extent are uncontrolled movements (arms/legs) present? (normal=5) a a se nsory skills 36 how well does he visually attend to a task? • a a 38 how well does student visually follow moving object? (tract) ab ab 39 how well does student visually select a preferred choice? (scan) ab ab μ cial/emotional behaviour 45 how often does self-stimulation occur? a a 46 how well does he make eye contact during communication? β a = case a; β = case β; ab = both cases a and β die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 lematic to score^han any other^ ^ w a ^ ^ ^ ^ t m v r r s n g w s t s in accordance with finds t s a m f a n " i e l d y k e (1988), who stated that the assessor should be skilled. when the scoring of the individual skill areas was studied, it became dear tha the teachers were too broadly trained in the skill areas to be tested with such a relatively fine measuring instrument. however, the repeated use of the rating scale is useful for monitoring progress in the classroom. in general, the items in the schedule were not significantly better scored after training. scoring is considered an advanced skill which requires more training. problematic questions in the schedule when both the ability to identify the items as well as the scoring of the schedule are taken into account, six questions were found to be difficult, of which four were found in the motor skill area of the schedule. of these only one question needs to be rephrased. difficult questions require specific attention in future training. conclusions the results indicated that the schedule is adequate for future use as teachers found it easy to use and the questions were adequately phrased. the training of teachers in the use of this schedule, however, needs to focus more on unfamiliar skill areas depending on the experience of the teacher (e.g., teachers who work with children with only cognitive disabilities would need more familiarisation with the motor skill area as their children are not physically disabled). the scoring routine of the schedule should be considered as an advanced skill which may need more practise and further training. the results obtained in this study highlight various issues which could have had an effect. this study showed that some children are more difficult to assess for specific teachers than others. two important factors which have to be kept in mind are: the teachers' familiarity with the disability: the majority of teachers in this workshop were unfamiliar with physically disabled children as they were used to working with primarily cognitively disabled children. the terminology applicable to the motor skill area was therefore especially at risk. this was pointed out as two thirds of the questions regarded as difficult occurred in the motor skill area. the effect of familiarity with the anna-marie wium and erna alant disability observed could be seen in their ability to identify items as well as in scoring. furthermore, the fact that the items which were poorly identified, and the items poorly scored did not match each other, supports the notion that the questions in the schedule are not so much poorly phrased, but that teachers were less familiar with certain skill areas due to their experience with specific disabilities. these results underline the complexity of assessing the severely disabled child (culp & carlyle, 1988), and also the fact that it is not easy to learn to observe objectively and in depth. it is therefore essential that future initial training workshops train a specific group of teachers with a familiar disability, as more reliable results could be obtained. additional disabilities can be introduced with more advanced follow-up courses in order to make teachers better observers. secondly, the severity of the disability: in addition to the above, teachers were also not familiar with the severity of the disabilities observed. the majority of teachers who attended this workshop had not been exposed to severely physically disabled children as their own . students were mainly severely or moderately cognitively disabled. familiarity in the use of the schedule and the ability to observe depends on repetition (stevens, 1978). this also became clear during training where it was observed that the more experienced and skilled teachers became in the use of the schedule , the less time was required to complete it and the more accurate their scoring became (as was continually noted in observation of case c). perhaps the most significant contribution of the schedule and the training was the effect it had on the selfconfidence of the teachers. the results in figure 1 indicated they felt confident of their ability to apply the schedule in future. this study has provided teachers with a screening tool as well as training in the use of the tool. the ability to effectively assess students by using the schedule made teachers feel efficient and in control. practise and continual use of the schedule will_ make them feel even more confident and efficient in assessment. this implies that teachers who are able to use the schedule effectively, become empowered to perform their role as teachers more successfully. most importantly in this study, the issue of a transdisciplinary approach was addressed. one of the problems in aac service delivery was that the responsibility of assessment had largely been considered as that of the professional speech therapists (hogg & raynes, 1987). such measures resulted in it being an exclusive service in this country, focusing on the community with the table 5: questions poorly scored no description of question skill areas 2 to what extent does he want to communicate with others? communication 6 how well can he point to an object (finger or hand)? communication 34 are any uncontrolled movements present? motor 36 how well does he visually attend to a task? sensory 41 how well does he attend auditory? sensory 45 how often does self-stimulation occur? social/emotional the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) l o o m e n t of a screening schedule for u s e b y the d e ^ o f children with severe disabilities a i ' ancial resources to afford it. this study provides a ! p n i n g instrument w h i c h can b e used b y a m u c h crroup of people, w h i c h in turn makes the service ΐ £ ι ι ^ β γ γ ν of aac more available and accessible to those work with children with severe disabilities. the f this schedule enables teachers to identify probγ δ β ° to refer to a specialised multi-disciplinary t e a m when necessary, to plan communication objectives and to monitor progress. this in turn results in more effective service delivery where problems such as limited manpower and funding are addressed. a c k n o w l e d g e m e n t s this study was funded b y the university of pretoria. r e f e r e n c e s american speech-hearing-language association (1992). (joint committee for the communicative needs of persons with severe disabilities). guidelines for meeting the communication needs of persons with severe disabilities. asha, j4w), barlow d & hersen, m. (1984). single-case experimental designs: strategies for studying behaviour change. oxford: pergamon press. . . . bereer a f presperin, j. & tallman, t. (1990). positioning for function: wheelchair and other technologies. new york: valhalla rehabilitations communications. beukelman, d.r. &.mirenda, p. (1992). augmentative and alternative communication: management of severe communication disorders in children and adults. baltimore: paul brookes publishing co. cuip, d. & carlyle, m. (1988). partners in augmentative teachers to d e s c r i b e the c o m m u n i c a t i o n 25 communication training. tuscon, arizona: communication skill builders. durant, v.m. (1990x severe behaviour problems: a functional communication training approach. london : the guilford hogg, j. & raynes, n. (1987). assessment in mental handicap: a guide to assessment practices, lists and checklists. london: croom helm. huer, m.b. (1983). the non speech ifest for receptive/expressive language. don johnston developmental equipment, inc, usa. kirkpatrick, e.g (1974). introductory statistics and probability for engineering, science and technology. in w.j. fabrycky, & j.h. mixe (eds.). new jersey: prentice-hall. light, j. (1989). toward a definition of communication competence for individuals using augmentative and alternative communication systems. aac, 5(2), 137-142. light, j., dattilo, j., english, j., gutierrez, l. & harts, j. (1992). instructing facilitators to support the communication of people who use augmentative communication systems. journal of speech and hearing research, august, 35, 865-875. mirenda, p. & iocono, t. (1990). communication options for persons with severe and profound disabilities: state of the art and future directions. journal of american speech and hearing, 15(1), 3-21. salvia, j. & yseldyke, j.e. (1988). assessment in special and remedial education. boston: houghton millin company. skuy, m., westaway, m., makula, n. & perold, c. (1988). development of a screening instrument for the identification of pupils with impairments. south african journal of education, 8(1), 45-49. stevens, m. (1978). observe then teach: an observational approach to teaching mentally handicapped children. london. edward arnold. wium, a.m. (1994). the development of a screening schedule for' teachers to describe the communication abilities of children with severe disabilities. unpublished m. log. thesis. university of pretoria. 0 play i x a n d m s c h o o l r o o m ! s h o p 6 l t h e r o s e b a n k m e w s 1 7 3 o x f o r d r o a d 1 r o s e b a n k j h b . tests, programmes, books, teaching aids, journals and resources for child development speech & language learning disabilities special needs p h o n e 788-1304 adult rehabilitation ^ s s i s w n i n social activities s a x o n w o l d f a x : 8 8 0 1 3 4 1 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 sonya warren, santie meyer and h.e.c. tesner afrikaa ns subjects english subjects south african signs 42% 35% variations voor dez a b in front cupped 1 finger extreme variation within compressed hand of) hand this sign compressed hand sig move hand point move hand across chest forward in finger forl-r or r-l semi-circle ward. wrist away from of dominant chest hand strikes wristof opposite hand tab chest neutral wrists chest orient verticalverticalverticaltoward body toward away from verticaltoward body body body face 50% 20% 87% na...toe dez a b (to) 1st finger 1st finger 1st finger 1st finger bilateral 1st finger sig move 1st point point finger join tips of fingers finger of finger join tips of fingers dominant hand along side of opposite 1st finger tab side of in front in front or l; r 1st finger of opposite hand opposite centre of body finger centre of body orient horizontal verticalvertical-unilateral opposite vertical-palm away palm down unilateral from body opposite face the use of signs and the coding of prefix markers by teachers at a school foi the deaf ι myrtle l aron, ph d (witwatersrand·) i robyn ε lewis, ba(log.) (witwatersrand) ba soc.science (unisa) j judy l willemse, ba (sp. & η therapy) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract the use of aspects of an artificially devised manual code in a black schoolfor the deaf was examined. the encoding of prefixes, bound with the noun class system, in tswana as used by seven teachers was studied as well as the consistency of the teachers to code lexical items. results .indicated the absence of signed prefix markers, inconsistency in signing lexical items and much variability among teachers in the signs used. the educational and research implications are discussed. /'' opsomming die gebruik van aspekte van "n kunsmatig ontwikkelde gebarestelsel in 'n swart skool vir dowes, is ondersoek. die enkodering van voorvoegsels verbonde aan die naamwoordklasstelsel in tswana soos gebruik deur sewe onderwysers is bestudeer, asook die konstantheid van die onderwyser se vermoe om leksikale items te kodeer. resultate dui op die afwesigheid van voorvoegselgebare, onkonstantheid van leksikale gebare en baie variasie onder onderwysers t. o. v. die gebare wat hulle gebruik. die opvoedkundige en navorsingsimplikasies word bespreek. ^ ο sasha 1986 77^ south african journal of communication disorders, vol 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) -fhe use of signs and the coding of prefix markers by teachers at a school for the deaf 65 the objective of deaf education has always been to teach the child the language of his culture — the culture of the hearing society in which he must live (bornstein, 1978; lewis 1984). this objective has pertained irrespective of the mode of communication used in in teaching deaf children. the long standing conflict between the use of oralism and manual communication still prevails although there has been a shift over the last twenty years in most education institutions for the deaf towards the use of manual communication often within a total communication framework. this shift has come about largely due to the apparent failure of oralism and oral education to teach spoken language and the increased use, interest and research into sign languages. total communication embraces an eclectic philosophy, which includes the combined use of a sign language system, manual coding, fingerspelling, speech, auditory training, speech reading, amplification, cueing and any other means whereby the child is taught to encode and decode language. a true natural sign language, which is the visual-gestural language of a deaf community, has its own lexicon, "phonological" and syntactic structure which is systematic and rule-governed. its basic encoding unit is the word represented by a sign. inflectional marking is distinguished by spatial temporal dimensions. nonmanual signs, such as stylised facial expression, also denote syntactic forms, such as question-type and subordinate clauses. (klima & belugi 1980; liddell, 1980; stokoe, 1978). it is expected that existing "natural" sign languages in south africa would show great divergence from each other due to the diverse ethnic, demographic and politically induced separation of social groups as well as differences in the spoken mother tongues. natural sign language plays a central role in maintaining the culture and ethnocentricity of deaf communities and is bound to the whole culture on one hand and physical constraints of the users on the other (stokoe 1978; cokeley and baker 1980; erting 1981). manual coding differs from natural sign in that it has been artifically devised by educationalists to represent the syntax of spoken language. in addition, some codes represent the morphological structure of the language in varying degrees (crystal et al. 1976; evans 1982). however, it is important to note that /tlie more reputable and widely used of these codes supplement the natural sign language, rather than replacing it. in order to relate sign language to coding, woodward (1972) suggested a sign continuum, with natural sign language at one end and the contrived sign systems at the other. a pidgin sign language occurs between the two extremes and incorporates elements of both signing and coding — it derives from the necessity for communication between deaf and hearing individuals. manual codes (mc) are used in educational settings and never as the home language of deaf people. examples of mcs include:signed english, manual english, seeing essential english (see) and signing exact english (see2), the rochester fingerspelling method and the paget-gorman sign system (pg). the latter system was developed in england and is of significance in south africa. it was first introduced at the kutlwanong school for the deaf in bophuthatswana some twenty-five years ago. no doubt it changed its form over the years of usage and undoubtedly provided part of the framework for the text talking to the deaf developed by nieder-heitmann past principal of kutlwanong school for some years. this text has now been introduced into all black schools for the deaf in southern africa. teaching staff are trained to use talking to the deaf by means of short courses and videotape (van der merwe, 1986). i the book talking to the deaf consists of 1500 signs which are claimed to be representative of signs used by the majority of deaf south africans. there is no empirical evidence or published data to support this claim. penn et al. (1984) provide preliminary evidence to show that these signs are not used by all deaf groups. the relationship of the nieder-heitmann (n-h) signs to natural sign languages is therefore much in question. the south african black languages are heavily morphological, being characterised by a noun class system and extensive agreement based upon that system. prefixes on the noun stem indicate noun class and number. th e prefix determines the form of the agreement morphemes of the other sentence constituents, all of which must be brought into agreement with the determining head noun (cole, 1982). although the n-h code makes some provisions for manually coding the syntax of black languages, this does not extend to the noun class system. according to gustason (1983) an effective code increases the input of morphological markers, word endings and other structural elements. it appears relevant and necessary to explore whether the use by teachers of the recently introduced dictionary of signs — talking to the deaf (nieder-heitmann, 1980) incorporates adaptations such as prefix markers intrinsic to tswana, the spoken language of the area. one black school for the deaf was selected for studying this. method aims of this study (1) to determine whether the teachers in a black school for the deaf are encoding certain prefixes of the tswana language, and if so, what form of coding they are using. (2) to examine the consistency with which the teachers are reproducing there prefix markers. (3) to ascertain whether the sign for a lexical item (noun) remains the same when used in isolation and in context by each teacher subject. (4) to note whether there is any influence of the signs from the original paget-gorman system on the signs used by the subjects for lexical items. seven subjects (a,b,c,d,e,f & g), all teachers at a black school for the deaf were selected. in addition, twenty-one pupils at the school were used to simulate a communicative interaction between teacher and pupil. the teacher subjects (ts's) were required to be native tswana speakers and to have taught at the school for at least two years so as to be familiar with the signing methods in use. all the ts's were teaching standards 1-5, in order to ensure that a sign system was used in the classroom, and that the test items were appropriate and meaningful. the pupils used to simulate a classroom situation were congenitally deaf, their home language was tswana and they had attended the school for a minimum of two years. they were therefore familiar with the signing system used during this time period. a national wv361 portable videocamera was used to record each ts's performance on the tasks. five tswana nouns, each from a different noun class and in singular and plural forms were selected. these nouns appear in talking to the deaf (nieder-heitmann 1980). the nouns selected were: noun class 1 moruti (teacher) noun class 2 mollo (five) noun class 3 leru (cloud) noun class 4 segokgo (spider) noun class 5 podi (goat) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 these lexical items were chosen for their frequency of use and relevance within the teaching situation. due to time limitations noun classes la, 6, 7, 8 and 9 were excluded. classes 1 to 4 take both singular and plural prefixes. multi-syllabic words in class 5 do not take a singular prefix (cole, 1982). this class was included . to ascertain whether coding of the singular occurs in spite of the absence of a prefix. as the plural prefixes of class 4 and 5 nouns are the same, it would be of interest to note whether they were coded differently. in addition, the object pronouns of each class were included in the test material as their grammatical form corresponds to the noun prefix. the object pronouns are as follows:noun class 1 ke a mo rata (i like him/her) noun class 2 ke a ο besa (i light it) noun class 3 ke a le bona (i see it) noun class 4 ke a se bolaya (i kill jt) noun class 5 ke a e gama (i milk jt) each ts was required to sign the selected nouns in the following forms: (1) singular — in isolation: moruti (teacher). (2) singular — in context: ke rata moruti (i like the teacher). (3) singular pronoun: ke a mo rata (i like him/her). (4) plural in isolation: baruti (teachers) (5) plural in context: ke rate baruti (i like the teachers). (6) plural pronoun: ke a ba rata (i like them). the subjects were video recorded signing the five nouns to the examiner (e) alone, and to three pupils. this was to ascertain whether the ts's would convey the prefix markers in an instructional setting with more deliberation. the nouns were signed in isolation and in context to see if they changed in form with varied syntactic contexts. at the end of the study the ts's were also required to complete a questionnaire concerning their awareness of the noun class system in tswana, and their need to convey such information to their pupils. in addition, information which might influence the ts's proficiency in sign usage was tapped, namely: teaching qualifications, length of teaching experience at the school, method of sign instruction, and perception of consistency of sign usage at the school. a questionnaire was also completed by the school principal, concerning the school's policy to signing, how the noun class system should be taught and the instruction of teachers in the system. analysis of data the signed nouns will be described, using the nieder-heitmann (n-h) code as reference. in addition, the paget-gorman signs will be considered in order to compare this original code with n-h and the ts's variations of the n-h code. each signed noun produced by the ts's was recorded as the same as the n-h system, or a variation (v) or as a totally different sign (d) to the n-h system. the variations or the different signs from the n-h were arranged in terms of the three cheremes described by stokoe (1976): a) dez (designator) — the configuration of the hands, b) tab (tabula) — the location on or near the body while the sign is made, (c) sig (signation) — the movement aspect of the hands. in addition, the signed noun in isolation (condition "a") was compared to its use in a syntactic context (condition "b") and described as the same (s) or different (d). myrtle l. aron, robyn e. lewis and judv l w „ 1 " w'"emse two graduate speech therapists acted as raters to analys sample of the teachers' signs to assess the accuracy of tl^ * v ' d e ° and categorization of all the signs as determined bv o r , a n a ' y s i s authors. e o f 'he results 1. signed nouns a. description of signs table 1 provides a description of the responses recorded denotes whether the signs used were the same as described^ nieder-heitmann (1980), or a variation thereof — the variaf ^ used are described in the table. table 2 provides a summa °of the data in table 1 and indicates the percentage scores of whethe the responses were nieder-heitmann signs, variations thereof totally different signs. it can be seen from both table 1 and table 2 that consistency i n signing between the ts's is minimal. only for the nouns "moruti" and "mollo" is there some overlap, where two ts's used the same variation. other variations are used exclusively by each ts however, many of the variations are minimal, differing in a single chereme. it can be noted from table 2 that a large percentage of signed nouns used are variations of the n-h signs (x80%). there was one occurrence of "moruti" which was totally different, and four occurrences for "podi". it is interestingto note that all ts's used the sign "preacher" rather than "teacher" (moruti). stokoe and kuschell (1979) note the significance of cultural factors in language and this may be an example of such cultural factors. comparing tables 1 and 3 it is clearly evident that the p-g signs differ extensively from both n-h and the ts's variations. reference will be made in the discussion, to the significance of the lack of similarity between the p-g and the teacher variations, although a study of the pupil's use of natural sign as compared with p-g would be of significant interest. b. consistency of use in isolation as compared to use in context there were two occurrences out of a total of seventy in which the signed nouns were different when used in isolation and context. the influence of syntactic context did not appear to affect the formation of the sign. however, as only a single syntactic context was used, this finding must be viewed with caution. writers such as stokoe (1976), and klima and bellugi (1980) have illustrated much variance in signing in context. signs may vary in terms of spatial and temporal dimensions. c. consistency of use in the demonstration of signs by the ts's to the ε as compared to the use of signs to pupils. there was also high consistency (80%) for the two situations. native users of sign language belong to a diglossic community, implying varieties of signing used by the same speaker under different conditions, particularly when addressing deaf or hearing individuals. as the ts's are not native users of sign, and only use it in the educational setting, this may explain the non-variance. ii prefix markers signed prefix markers were not used by the ts's for any of the test items. this fining indicates that no manual means exist in the signing system for coding the variety of noun classes in tswana. this was substantiated by the ts's answers to the questionnaire. five ts's reported that the noun class system is not taugn formally, but is deduced by the pupils from the written form. τ 1 s written form is recorded on a wall-chart, used for learning dn the school principal was uncertain as to how the noun pretixe the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) n>« nh the coding of prefix markers by teachers at a school for the deaf u s e of sign s anu 67 13v • t on of the variations with dez, tab and sig formations of the lexical items used by the teacher subjects as compared to f»ble 1 ^ ^ e r h e i t m a n n reference signs ( ii) variations: bilateral: th&l-f positioned approximately 3cm apart, b of hands face body. 1. unilateral 2. bilateral: not symmetrical 1-h; 1-f points up, th & 2-f slightly apart point horizontally 2-h: as for n-h sign. 3. bilateral: th & 1-f further apart, ρ faces body 4. unilateral: 1-f and then tab sig no. of subjects using signs in the 2 situations ts to ε ts to pupils neck/throat 2. mollo (i)n-h sign: (ii) variations: 3. leru (i) n-h sign: (ii) variations: bilateral: spread-h, ρ faces body 1. unilateral 2. compressed-h 3. compressed-h 4. unilateral: compressed-h 1-h:side of face 2-h as for n-h sign chest neck and chest. chest 5. — bilateral: spread-h; b. of hand faces floor abdomen level with forehead i 1. — 2. unilateral: flat-h; ρ faces floor. 3. cupped-h; p. faces floor 4. unilateral: flat-h; p. faces floor 5. unilateral: clawed-h; p. faces floor 6. unilateral: p. faces floor 7. unilateral: flat-h p. faces floor 8. unilateral: flat-h above head above head. move hands apart from midline to edge of neck indicating collar. 1 (e) 2 (f&g) move hands across from 2 (a;c) 1 (a) one side of neck to the other 1-h stationary 0 1 (b) 2-h: move hand across from one side of neck to the other move hands apart from 2 (c:d) 2 (c:d) midline to edge of chest. move 1-f across neck, then 1 (f) 1 (f) flat-h down chest. move hands up and down and repeated tapping movement of fingers indicating flames. tapping movement of fingers against th no up and down hand movement. tapping movement of fingers against th only upward hand movement. 2 (a:g) 2 (a:g) 1 (b) 2 (c:e) 1 (d) 1 (f) move hands apart from 0 midline, using pronounced semi-circular movements along a horizontal plane. hands positioned adjacent 1 (a) each other and move in same direction across body moves hand shorter 1 (b) distance along horizontal plane; very slight semicircular movements. very slight semi-circular 1 (c) hand movement. moves hand shorter 1 (d) distance along horizontal plane; very slight semicircular movements. moves hand along horizon1 (e) tal plane: initial semicircular movements very slight. moves hand along horizon1 (f) tal plane; rapid and very slight semi-circular movement. 1 (g) fingers move independently 0 1 (b) 2 (c:e) 1 (d) 1(f) 0 1 (a) 1 (b) 1 (c) 1 (d) 1 (e) 1 (f) 0 1 (c) d" suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 myrtle l. aron, robyn e. lewis and judy l. willemse 4, segokgo (i) n-h sign: unilateral: clawed-h; level with nose slow vertical movement 0 0 p. faces floor. from centre/neutral position to position level with nose; then slight horizontal movement, fingers move rapidly as if typing. (ii) variations: 1. bilateral chest hands across at wrist; 1 (a) 1 (a) no vertical movement of hands; fingers move in unison tapping against th. 2. — — hands move diagonally 1 (b) 1 (b) from side of chest to above head. 3. — neck/throat slight horizontal hand 1 (c) 1 (c) movement absent. 4. — — hand moves across chest: 1 (d) 0 no vertical movement of hand upwards. 5. cupped-h. — no finger movement; 0 1 (d) no vertical movement of hand upwards. 6. — — rapid vertical movement; 1 (e) 1 (e) slight horizontal movement absent. 7. — chest no vertical movement 1 (f) 1 (f) of hand upwards. 8. b. of hand faces body — vertical movement begins 1 (g) 1 (g) in upper chest region, slight horizontal movement absent. 5. podi (i) n-h sign: unilateral; fist-h; neck/throat hand moves from chin a 1 (g) 1 (g) knuckles face away short distance vertically from body downwards, indicating beard. (ii) variations: 1. th, 1-f and 2-f together — hand moves from chin in 1 (a) 1 (a) semi-circle outwards. 2. bilateral; not r.h. top of head r.h. — stationary 1 (c) 0 symmetrical l.h.-neck/throat l.h. rapid movements up r.h. — v-h and down from chin. l.h. — th, 1-f and 2-f together. i 3. same as 2. same as 2. r.h. — stationary 0 1 (c) ! l.h. — one slow movement i down from chin. 4. th (on top of chin) — slow downward movement 1 (e) 1(e) 1 and 1-f (under chin) against chin twice s. same as 4. — rapid downwards 1 (f) 1 (f) movement against chin. key: f — finger th — thumb b — back ρ — palm 1-f — first finger 2-f — second finger 3-f — third finger 1-h — first hand 2-h — second hand r.h. — right hand l.h. — left hand flat-h — flat-hand spread-h — spread-hand compressed-h — compressed hand cupped-h — cupped-hand clawed-h — clawed-hand fist-h — fist-hand v-h — v-hand the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) he use of signs and the coding of prefix markers by teachers at a school for the deaf 69 ble 2 signed nouns used by each teacher subject in terms of whether they are nieder-heitmann signs, variations thereof, or totally τ 3 * different from the nieder-heitmann sign, and number and percentage of occurrence within each category in both situations across all nouns. situation sign n-h v d tss situation moruti mollo leru segokgo podi no/10 % no/10 % no/10 % a + v v n-h n-h v v v v v v 2 20 8 80 — — β + d v v v v v v v d d — — 7 70 3 30 c + v v v v v v v v v v — — 10 100 — — d + v v v v v v v v d d — — 8 80 2 20 ε + n-h n-h v v v v v v v v 2 20 8 80 — — f + v v v v v v v v v v — — 10 100 — — — v n-h v v n-h 5 50 5 50 _ + n-h n-h v v n-h x 9 13 56 80 5 7 key: — = ts demonstration to e; + = ts demonstration to group of pupils; n-h v = variation of n-h sign; d = totally different from n-h sign = nieder-heitmann sign; table 3 description of paget-gorman signs within dez, tab and sig formations (nieder-heitmann 1980) sign dez tab sig teacher (moruti) (no sign for preach/er) 1-h: 1-f pt up, tl & 2nd finger pt horizontally. 2-h: compressed hand 1-h level with shoulder 2-h-side forehead 1-h: extend tl & 1 finger outwards. 2-h-twist wrist to open hand fire: (mollo) (unilateral) 5 point-hand, (fingers & thumb pt upwardstraight & seperate from one another) side of chest move all digits simultaneously & independently of one another cloud (leru) unilateral, spread-hand, palm faces floor forehead vibrate hand vertically segohgo (spider) unilateral clawed hand p. faces floor shoulder hand lowers vertically, one hand length moving digits independantly podi (goat) 1-h; l f & 4-f up; 2-f & 3-f on th. palm faces floor. 2-h: 1-f on side 4-f. 1-h & th on side 2-f 1-h 1-h: shoulder 2-h: lower v2h. breadth closing th & 1-f. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 myrtle l. aron, robyn e. lewis and judy l. willemse table 4 description of the nieder-heitmann object pronoun sign across the cheremic configurations, and variations thereof sign dez tab sig n-h sign: unilateral; 1-f points diagonally downwards. side of body (same side as ! hand involved) stationary variations: dez 1: bilateral dez 2: f points straight or diagonally upwards. tab 1: f points to front of body. tab 2: f points above head. sig 1: hand moves across body, sig. 2: m table 5 description of the nieder-heitmann plural sign across the cheremic configurations, and variations thereof sign dez tab sig n-h sign: unilateral: th and 1-f meet. side of chest below shoulder of opposite side. move hand across chest from" shoulder on same side to lower position below shoulder of opposite side. variations: tab 1: mid-chest area. tab 2: lower face/neck area. tab 3: side of body opposite face. tab 4: mouth tab 5: shoulder of same side. sig 1: no movement of hand from shoulder of same side to other shoulder. sig 2: hand moves from midline of chest to opposite shoulder. sig 3: hand moves from shoulder to midline of chest. were taught to the pupils, but believed the teachers used the written mode. a single ts stated that she finger-spelled the prefixes and another emphasized the role of lip-reading. five ts's stated that no instruction had been given to them on how to convey noun classes. (the remaining two ts's appeared to miscomprehend the question, and responded inappropriately). ill the object pronoun and plural forms as indicated above the noun prefixes for both structures were not distinguished through manual means. they were illustrated purely as lexical items by their respective n-h signs or variations thereof, as were the singular nouns. the most interesting variations occurred for the object pronoun. the n-h sign does not account for a location shift, according to the direction in space appropriate to the person or object being referred to. however, in the variations used by some ts's (see table 4) it appears that there was an attempt to increase the morphological information. this is in accordance with pronominalisation in american sign language as well as various mce systems (gustason 1980). the n-h sign employs the non-manual component of eye direction — the signer is required to look at his hand when signing; presumably to indicate who is being referred to. this was observed in only two of the ts's. the n-h plural sign and variations thereof are described in table 5. it can be seen that there was a great deal of variation in both the tab and the sig cheremes. signs changed position in front of the body, in close conjunction with the context sign (schlesinger, 1978), and this principle was clearly seen operating here. a general trend was that the formation of the plural marker was less discrete in syntactic context than in isolation. iv results of the questionnaire put to teacher subjects 1) a single ts had a diploma in special education — the others had only a general primary teacher's diploma. evans (1982) feels that professional education of teachers of the deaf should provide for instruction in manual communication during a specialised period of study. 2) the period of service at the school ranged from 6 to 12 years. this suggests that all the ts's had been exposed to a signing system which differed from n-h for several years prior to its introduction. it would thus be expected that some confusion with the "old' system would arise, particularly if it is used by the children as their natural sign system. 3) the ts's stated they had been taught "the signs" by the more exerienced teachers at the school, as well as from the pupils and by using the text talking to the £> + \3500 / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 200 2nfatcc0s( g(f,a) } ( 1 ) where g(f,a) is the synchronization index, k is a scaling factor used to fit neurophysiological data, f is the stimulus frequency, and a is the stimulus intensity. this equation was derived in hanekom and kruger (2001). the synchronization index g(f,a) is a function of both frequency and intensity. g(f,a) may be written as the product of two factors, g(f,a) = g/f) g/a), where a is intensity in db sl and fis frequency in hz. g/β and g/a) characterize the variation of the synchronization index with variation in frequency and intensity of the pure tone stimulus respectively. for acoustic stimulation g/f) is given by ι + \3500j and g/a) is given by 1.1 a°m g/a) = 0.6 (3) v0.5(a 0 3 ) 2 i p + k equation 2 and equation 3 are curve fits to typical values of the synchronization index as a function of frequency and intensity respectively. in equation 3, if is a sensitivity constant that controls the threshold of the model fibre, while η is a tuning constant that takes on a maximum value of 1 when the model fibre has cf at the stimulus frequency. model of phase-locking for electrical frequency ( h z ) figure 1. synchronization index as a function of frequency for electrical and acoustic stimulation. the solid curve (electrical stimulation) was calculated from equation 4. the dotted curve (acoustic stimulation) was calculated from equation 2. open squares are data from johnson (1980) and open circles are data from javel and mott (1988) for acoustic stimulation. filled circles are data for sinusoidal electrical stimulation from dynes and delgutte (1992). r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) what do cochlear implants teach us about the encoding of frequency in the auditory system? 53 stimulation may in fact disperse spikes around the preferred phase in electrical stimulation as measured at a central integration centre. so for electrical stimulation, it is assumed that phase-locked spike trains arriving at the central integration centre are desynchronized relative to each other. it is assumed that the central integration centre still generates one spike per stimulus cycle, but with larger variance in spike position around the preferred phase than for the acoustic case. the spike position variance is a function of the number of spike trains combined. it is assumed that the spike trains arriving at the central integration centre are statistically independent, so that the spike variances add. if it is assumed that spike trains from around 200 fibres are combined, the spike variance around the preferred phase will be two hundred times larger for electrical stimulation than for acoustic stimulation. thus an = v200 ^k -ί-τarccos ( g i ^ ° · 5 ) ( 5 ) 2 π/ ctjc/j is used for electrical stimulation, with g/β as given in equation 4. as further motivation for this argument, it is noted that injuries to the auditory nerve cause increases in neural conduction time (and thus temporal dispersion of neural activity). it is known that injuries to the auditory nerve affect speech discrimination ability more than cochlear injuries (moller, 1999). implementation of the estimator and simulations stimulus of duration τ many (typically 200) times and calculating the standard deviation of the frequency estimate at a specific time. this time was either at the end of the interval τ or after 50 observations of inter-spike intervals, as will be explained in the discussion. values of af were obtained as a function of stimulus frequency for both acoustic and electrical stimulation. the equations describing the model were coded in matlab, a computer language designed for doing mathematics. simulations were run on a pentium ii personal computer under the windows 95 operating system. results af/f as a function of frequency for acoustic stimulation figure 2 shows the normalized frequency difference limen (af/f) as a function of frequency for acoustic stimulation as predicted by the model. for comparison, af/f for electrical stimulation is also shown. frequency discrimination data for acoustic stimulation as measured by sek and moore (1995) are plotted on the same axis. the shapes of the two curves for acoustic stimulation are very similar, and both reach minima at 500 hz. the absolute values of af/f as predicted by the model correspond well to measured values across the entire frequency range, except at 10000 hz. af as a function of freq uency for electrical stimulation derivation of the kalman filter equations falls outside the scope of this article, but details may be found in hanekom and kriiger (2001). the discussion below is intended to elucidate the principles. essentially, to apply the kalman filter, the modelling equations for the generation of spikes must be obtained in a certain format (the state space format, which is often used by engineers). this is relatively simple for the problem stated 'here. once this has been done, the noisy measurements of the actual stimulus period may be applied as input to the kalman filter. from an implementation viewpoint, the kalman filter is then simply a set of equations solved iteratively to provide an estimate' for the input frequency at each time instant. i spike trains from single fibres were computer generated using the> model. estimates were obtained for frequency by observing the spike train from a single modelled fibre under the assumption that one spike per stimulus cycle was available. spikes were placed according to a gaussian distribution with standard deviation σ η . for acoustic stimulation, g/f) of equation 2 was used in equation 1 to calculate ση, while g/f) as in equation 4 was used in equation 5 for electrical stimulation. the frequency difference limen af was then obtained by assuming it to be equal to the standard deviation in the frequency estimate, following siebert (1970) and several other authors after him. the standard deviation in the frequency estimate was obtained by repeating the pure tone c ω j ω ο c ω ι— ω τ3 ω ν "to ε ιο ζ figure 3 shows the frequency difference limen (af) as a function of frequency for electrical stimulation as predicted by the model. simulation predictions are not shown as the 0 . 0 8 0.04 0.02 0.01 0.008 0.004 0.002 0.001 -ι 1 100 200 500 1000 2000 5000 8000 frequency(hz) figure 2. values of the frequency difference limen af expressed as a proportion of frequency (aflf) are plotted as a function of the frequency of a pure tone stimulus on logarithmic axes. filled squares are model predictions for acoustic stimulation, while open circles are the perceptual frequency discrimination data of sek and moore (1995). filled circles are model predictions for electrical stimulation. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 johan j. hanekom weber fraction af/fin this figure, as the measurement data was available as af. frequency discrimination data as documented in pfingst (1988) and ibwnshend et al. (1987) are plotted on the same axes. the shapes and slopes of the model prediction and the psychoacoustic data are similar. the absolute values of af as predicted by the model are smaller than measured values across most of the frequency range. at high frequencies, model predictions may be an order smaller than the psychoacoustic data. to bring the model and data into closer agreement, larger standard deviations may be used for spike position jitter. alternatively, higher stimulus intensities shift the measured curves downwards (pfingst, 1988). unfortunately, it is unknown at which stimulus intensities the data were measured. discussion justification of assumptions the current model rests on two important assumptions. first, it was assumed that the auditory system has some way to ensure one spike per stimulus interval across the entire frequency range. this assumption idealizes known neurophysiological data. more than one spike may occur per stimulus cycle in acoustic stimulation (rose, brugge, anderson & hind, 1968) and multiple spikes may occur in electrical stimulation (javel & shepherd, 2000). both phases of the electrical stimulation waveform may evoke spikes (van den honert & stypulkowski, 1987) and multiple spikes per phase may occur at higher frequencies of pulsatile stimulation (javel & shepherd, 2000). because of the way that the current model was formulated, it is a requirement that there is only one spike per stimulus cycle. the kalman filter will regard more than one spike per stimulus interval as a source of noise. if a small percentage of cycles have either more than one spikes per cycle, or some cycles are skipped, the dominant inter-spike interval is still the period of the stimulus waveform and the central estimator will make the correct estimate (although with larger standard deviation in the estimate). with many cycles not conforming to the one spike per cycle assumption, the central estimator may make an incorrect estimate of the input frequency. a higher likelihood exists that this will happen for electrical stimulation, as spikes may occur on both phases of the stimulus waveform. nonetheless, the model may still explain the observed frequency difference limens, because frequency discrimination measurements are differential and do not measure the absolute frequency perceived. the close correlation between the predicted and measured frequency discrimination thresholds suggests the possibility that a central representation of the pure tone exists that is equivalent to the one spike per stimulus interval assumption. this, however, is not what the model intended to prove. rather, the intention was to show that frequency discrimination thresholds could be explained by spike position jitter in a phaselocked response. this is discussed further in the sequel. the second assumption was that, because many fibres fire in phase as a result of electrical stimulation, the net result at the central auditory estimator would be a desynchronization of spike trains, rather than improved synchronization. it is unknown whether data exist which supports this hypothesis. available data seems to refute this notion. the cochlear nucleus (cn) exhibits greater response diversity than the auditory nerve (o'leary, tong & clark, 1995). some fibres display phase-locking to the stimulus, while the responses of other fibres are more complex. cn fibres that do phaselock exhibit very little temporal dispersion of spikes for electrical stimulation (javel & shepherd, 2000). however, as it is not known what the central representation of frequency is, to search for spike trains at the cn output that exhibits larger spike position jitter for electrical stimulation than for acoustic stimulation may be fallacious. it is known that temporal information on the auditory nerve is gradually transformed into a rateplace code at higher levels of the central auditory system, possibly at the level of the cn (rhode and greenberg, 1992). many auditory afferents carrying a phase-lock code converge on cn cells. these fibres should provide at least one spike per stimulus cycle on the input to a cn neuronal assembly. the possibility exists that the phase-lock code may then be transformed directly into a rate-place code without the need for fibres firing at rates up to 5000 hz. so, not enough is known to be able to prove or disprove the second assumption. neither assumption is unrealistic in terms of biological implementation and the results justify the two assumptions to some extent. as a final comment, the possibility that the model predictions only hold for frequencies below 5000 hz needs to be pointed out, as no phase-locking is observed at higher (acoustic) stimulation frequencies. ν ~ 1000 ^ 500 c (u ε ω ο c ω l_ ω ifc > ο c ω ^ cγ ω 100 50 10 100 300 600 1000 3000 6000 10000 f r e q u e n c y ( h z ) figure 3. values of the frequency difference limen δf are plotted as a function of the frequency of electrical stimulation., on logarithmic axes. filled circles are model predictions for electrical stimulation. open circles and open diamonds are perceptual frequency discrimination data from two studies (for sinusoidal electrical stimulation) as reported in pfingst (1988). open squares are data for pulsatile electrical stimulation (townshend et al., 1987). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) what do cochlear implants teach us about the encoding of frequency in the auditory system? 55 the origin of the shape of the af/f frequency curve the af obtained is primarily a tradeoff between two parameters of the model: the number of observations and the spike jitter around the preferred phase of the stimulus cycle. to account for psychoacoustic data below 500 hz, stimulus duration γ is limited to 100 ms so that the number of observations decreases with lower frequencies, which results in a growth in af/f at lower frequencies consistent with psychoacoustic data. this choice for τ is consistent with known auditory integration times (eddins & green, 1995). at these frequencies, af/f is determined primarily by the number of observations available. at higher frequencies the number of observations in the 100 ms time interval grows. it was found that the number of observations needs to be close to n= 50 to achieve the same af/f values as the psychoacoustic data for acoustical stimulation. larger ν results in little further decrease in aflf. at higher frequencies (above 500 hz), the spike jitter becomes a systematically growing percentage of the stimulus period. this plays the primary role in the growth of aflf at these frequencies. what do cochlear implants teach us about the coding of frequency in the auditory system? psychoacoustic data from cochlear implants seem to refute the idea that temporal coding mechanisms are utilized by the central auditory system to extract frequency information from the neural spike train, as the frequency difference limens are much poorer than for normal-hearing listeners, even though there is much more synchronization to the stimulus waveform in electrical stimulation. the current model demonstrates (with reasonable assumptions) that a central auditory estimator that uses interspike intervals to calculate frequency may fare worse with electrical stimulation than with acoustic stimulation. this is consistent with psychoacoustic data. so at least the .current model indicates ,'that we cannot rule out temporal mechanisms as a mechainism for frequency coding. it is known that cochlear implant signal processing strategies based on preserving the temporal pattern (e.g. cis) are generally more successful than strategies based on vocoders (e.g. speak) (loizou, 1999), which supports the argument in favour jof phase-lock coding. also, recent studies have shown that fewer channels in a speech processor can lead to equally good or better speech discrimination (fishman, shannon & slattery, 1997), but if fewer than 4 to 6 channels are used, performance drops. the interpretation is that the actual number of independent information channels in an implant is probably not more than 4 to 6. also, because higher stimulation rates can be achieved with fewer activated electrodes (shannon, adams, ferrel, palumbo & grandgenett, 1990), the temporal characteristics of the signal are preserved better. thus, evidence suggests that good spatial resolution is not achieved in cochlear implants, but also that preservation of the temporal waveform is important in cochlear implants. conversely, it has been shown in many pitch discrimination or electrode discrimination experiments (nelson, van tasell, schroder, soli & levine, 1995; pfingst, holloway, zwolan & collins, 1999), where a fixed stimulation frequency was used on various electrodes, that cochlear implant users can discriminate between electrodes. furthermore, pitch estimation experiments show that implant users can assign pitch to electrodes in a systematic fashion (dorman, smith, smith & parkin, 1994) which follows the tonotopical arrangement of the cochlea. spikes are entrained to the stimulus in electrical stimulation (javel, 1990), so if the phase-lock code was the only mechanism operating in frequency discrimination or pitch perception, stimuli on all electrodes would have had the same pitch. so electrode discrimination and pitch estimation experiments provide convincing arguments in favour of the rate-place code. it is concluded that cochlear implants have not yet provided the final answers to the question of the coding of frequency in the auditory system. implications for cochlear implants it is far easier to get high temporal resolution in electrical stimulation than it is to get high spectral resolution. current spread from electrodes limit spectral resolution (krai, hartmann, mortazavi & klinke, 1998). new electrode designs may limit current spread (cords, reuter, issing, sommer, kuzma & lenarz, 2000), but certain physical limitations on electrode design remain. for example, maximum safe levels of charge density exist (shannon, 1992). on the other hand, there are no basic technological limitations on increasing the stimulation rate. however, neural threshold adaptation may occur for high stimulation rates (above 400 pulses per second per channel), which suggests that higher stimulation rates may not be beneficial and may even degrade speech recognition performance (javel & shepherd, 2000). still, the success of temporal pattern based strategies for cochlear implants like cis is encouraging and warrants further study. conclusions (1) to be able to predict frequency difference limens for acoustic stimulation, an important assumption is that one spike per stimulus cycle is available, which may be provided by the existence of a volley principle. the volley principle may be implemented by the cochlear nucleus, where neurons have been found that can improve temporal precision by combination of a number of auditory nerve inputs (moller, 1999). (2) an additional assumption is required in order to predict frequency difference limens for electrical stimulation of the auditory system. it is assumed that because many fibres fire on exactly the same phase of the electrical stimulation waveform, desynchronization results at a central auditory nervous system integration centre, which in turn leads to degradation in frequency discrimination. (3) psychoacoustic data from cochlear implants show that both mechanisms for the coding of frequency information in the auditory system are equally likely. thus, though cochlear implants may provide a tool to solve this problem, they have not yet provided the final answer to the question of coding of frequency in the auditory system. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 johan j. hanekom references blarney, p.j., dooley, g.j., parisi, e.s., & clark, g.m. (1996). pitch comparisons of acoustically and electrically evoked auditory sensations. hearing research, 99, 139-150. cook, e.p. & johnston, d. (1999). voltage-dependent properties of dendrites that eliminate location-dependent variability of synaptic input. journal of neurophysiology, 81, 535-543. cords, s.m., reuter, g., issing, pr., sommer, α., kuzma, j. & lenarz, t. (2000). a silastic positioner for a modiolus-hugging position of intracochlear electrodes: electrophysiologic effects. american journal of otology, 21, 212-217. dorman, m.f., smith, m., smith, l., & parkin, j.l. (1994). the pitch of electrically presented sinusoids. journal of the acoustical society of america, 95, 1677-1679. dynes, s.b.c. & delgutte, b. (1992). phase-locking of auditorynerve discharges to sinusoidal electric stimulation of the cochlea. hearing research, 58, 79-90. eddins, d.a., green, d.m. (1995). temporal integration and temporal resolution. in b.c.j. moore (ed.), hearing. san diego: academic press. fishman, k., shannon, r.v., & slattery, w.h. (1997). speech recognition as a function of the number of electrodes used in the speak cochlear implant speech processor. journal of speech and hearing research, 40, 1201-1215. goldstein, j.l. & srulovicz, p. (1977). auditory-nerve spike intervals as an adequate basis for aural frequency measurement. in e.f. evans & j.p. wilson (eds.), psychophysics and physiology of hearing. london: academic press. hanekom, j.j. and kruger, j.j. (2001). a model of frequency discrimination with optimal processing of auditory nerve spike intervals. hearing research, 151, 188-204. hartmann, r., topp, g., & klinke, r. (1984). electrical stimulation of the cat cochlea discharge pattern of single auditory fibres. advances in audiology, 1, 18-29. javel, e. (1990). acoustic and electrical encoding of temporal information. in j.m. miller and f.a. spelman (eds.), cochlear implants: models of the electrically stimulated ear, new york: springer-verlag. javel, e. & mott, j.b. (1988). physiological and psychophysical correlates of temporal processes in hearing. hearing research, 34, 275-294. javel, e. & shepherd, r.k. (2000). electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve. iii. response initiation sites and temporal fine structure. hearing research, 140, 45-76. johnson, d.h. (1980). the relationship betweeen spike rate and synchrony in responses of auditory-nerve fibers to single tones. journal of the acoustical society of america, 68, 11151122. johnstone, b.m., patuzzi, r., & yates, g.k. (1986). basilar membrane measurements and the travelling wave. hearing research, 22, 147-153. kalman, r.e. (1960). a new approach to linear filtering and prediction problems. transactions of the asme journal of basic engineering, 82, 35-45. kim, d.o. & parham, k. (1991). auditory nerve spatial encoding of high-frequency pure tones: population response profiles derived from d' measure associated with nearby places along the cochlea. hearing research, 52, 167-180. krai, α., hartmann, r., mortazavi, d., & klinke, r. (1998). spatial resolution of cochlear implants: the electrical field and excitation of auditory afferents. hearing research, 121, 11-28. loizou, p.c. (1999). signal-processing techniques for cochlear implants. ieee engineering in medicine and biology magazine, 18, 34-46. moller, a.r. (1999). review of the roles of temporal and place coding of frequency in speech discrimination. acta otolaryngology (stockholm), 119, 424-430. moore, b.c.j. (1973). frequency difference limens for shortduration tones. journal of the acoustical society of america 54, 610-619. moore, b.c.j. & glasberg, b.r. (1986). the role of frequency selectivity in the perception of loudness, pitch and time. in b.c.j. moore (ed.), frequency selectivity in hearing. london: academic press. nelson, d.a., van tasell, d.j., schroder, a.c., soli, s., & levine, s. (1995). electrode ranking of "place pitch" and speech recognition in electrical hearing. journal of the acoustical society of america, 98, 1987-1999. o'leary, s.j., tbng, y.c., & clark, g.m. (1995). responses of dorsal cochlear nucleus single units to electrical pulse train stimulation of the auditory nerve with a cochlear implant electrode. journal of the acoustical society of america, 97, 2378-2393. pfingst, b.e. (1988). comparisons of psychophysical and neurophysiological studies of cochlear implants. hearing research, 34, 243-252. pfingst, b.e., holloway, l.a., zwolan, t.a., & collins, l.m. (1999). effects of stimulus level on electrode-place discrimination in human subjects with cochlear implants. hearing research, 134, 105-115. rhode, w.s., greenberg, s. (1992). physiology of the cochlear nuclei. in a.n. popper & r.r. fay (eds.), the mammalian auditory pathway: neurophysiology. new york: springerverlag. rose, j.e., brugge, j.f., anderson, d.j., & hind, j.e. (1968). patterns of activity in single auditory nerve fibres of the squirrel monkey. in a.v.s. de reuck & j. knight, hearing mechanisms in vertebrates. london: j & a churchill ltd. sachs, m.b. & miller, m.i. (1985). pitch coding in the auditory nerve: possible mechanisms of pitch sensation with cochlear implants. in r.a. schindler & m.m. merzenich (eds.), cochlear implants. new york: raven press. sek, a. & moore, b.c.j. (1995). frequency discrimination as a function of frequency, measured in several ways. journal of the acoustical society of america, 97, 4, 2479-2486. shannon, r.v. (1992). a model of safe levels for electrical stimulation. ieee transactions on biomedical engineering, 39,424426. shannon, r.v., adams, d.d., ferrel, r.l., palumbo, r.l., & grandgenett, m. (1990). a computer interface for psychophysical and speech research with the nucleus cochlear implant. journal of the acoustical society of america, 87, 905-907. shepherd, r.k. & javel, e. (1999). electrical stimulation of the ausditory nerve: ii. effect of stimulus waveshape on single fibre response properties. hearing research, 130, 171-188. siebert, w.m. (1970). frequency discrimination in the auditory system: place or periodicity mechanisms? proceedings of the ieee, 58, 723-730. srulovicz, p. & goldstein, j.l. (1983). a central spectrum model: a synthesis of auditorynerve timing and place cues in monaural communication of frequency spectrum. journal; of the acoustical society of america, 73, 1266-1276. ; townshend, b., cotter, n., van compernolle, d., & white, r:l. (1987). pitch perception by cochlear implant subjects. journal of the acoustical society of america, 82, 106-115. i van den honert, c. & stypulkowski, p.h. (1987). temporal response patterns of single auditory nerve fibers elicited by periodic electrical stimuli. hearing research, 29, 207-222. ι wakefield, g.h. & nelson, d.a. (1985). extension of a temporal model of frequency discrimination: intensity effects in normal and hearing-impaired listeners. journal of the acoustical society of america, 77, 613-619. wever, e.g. (1949). theory of hearing. new york: wiley. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 93 community based education in speech pathology and audiology at the university of durban-westville in an under served community* glen w jager department of speech and hearing therapy university of durban-westville abstract in south africa there is increasing awareness both in academic and clinical domains of the inadequacy of education and training of health personnel, in preparing graduates to meet the service needs of the disadvantaged majority. a feasibility study is reported in which the regular curriculum of final year speech, language therapy i audiology students was adapted to provide a more relevant, more appropriate learning experience. this comprised a community based action research programme in a zulu peri-rural community. a qualitative critical analysis of the project is presented, in an attempt to identify factors that could be likely to hinder and promote the greatly needed process of curriculum transformation within the university; and in the process of strengthening the role that the university can play in meeting the needs of the community it serves. opsomming daar is 'n toenemende bewuswording in suid afrika, op beide die akademiese en kliniese gebied ten opsigte van die ontoereikende opvoedkundige en gesondheidspersoneel, om gegradueerdes voor te berei om aan die diensverskaffmgseise van die minderbevoorregte meerderheid te voldoen. 'n vatbaarheidsstudie word uiteengesit waarin die normale kurrikulum van finalejaar spraaktaalterapieen oudiologiestudente aangepas is om 'n meer relevante en toepaslike leersituasie daar te stel. dit het 'n praktiese gemeenskapsgebaseerde navorsingsprogram· in 'n peri-landelike zulugemeenskap behels. 'n kwalitatiewe kritiese analise van die projek is voorgestel, in 'n poging om faktore te identifiseer wat aanduidings verskaf om die nodige prosesveranderings van die kurrikulum in die universiteit te bewerkstellig, asook die bef^lemtoning van die rol wat die universiteit kan speel om te voldoen aan die gemeenskapseise. the development of health personnel able and willing to serve the community by providing health care, promoting healthij preventing disease and caring for those in need is a major and formidable task for educators. | h. mahler, director-general of the world health-organisation (who), (cited by lipkin, 1989a). vative community based; problem based medical education; in current thinking about the role of the university in the community it serves; and in increasing commitment to the principles of primary health care (phc) as defined in the world health organisation report on the international conference on primary health care at alma-ata (1978). 2 background 1 introduction in south africa, in both academic and clinical domains, there is increasing awareness of the'inadequacy of education and training of health personnel in preparing graduates to meet the service needs of the disadvantaged majority. this is reflective not only of the current political changes but also of progressive thinking in education; in international trends towards innoin order to contextualise this growing concern with the inadequacies of the status quo in health personnel education, the paradigms, to which dynamic influences can be attributed, must be expanded. 2.1 educational background the educational concepts of a participatory, interactive, problem solving, teaching/learning paradigm are * the original version of this paper was presented at the third international symposium on the role of universities in developing areas, 7-9 july 1993, university of the azores at ponta delgada, s. miguel, in conjunction with the university of california, los angeles, and ben-gurion university, beer-sheeva. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 94 glen w. jager not new, nor are they restricted to the discipline of eduhealth for all, is to implement the principles of phc. cation. since classical times, following the thinking of based as it is on a biopsychosocial model of health, phc, socrates, there has been an acknowledgement of the encompasses social, psychological as well as physical significance of self discovery. this has formed the badimensions of well-being. a fully comprehensive apsis for theorists such as bruner (1966), and rogers proach to health care is thus implied, and focuses, inter (1969) (lipkin, 1989b). the problem based learning alia, on the attainment of basic needs. people, and their approach, which is based upon the case study method right to choose and to decide about their own health, (fraser 1931) used at the harvard law school and the need to be respected and accounted to by sensitive, comdiscovery learning approach, was developed at the passionate service providers. they need to take control mcmaster university (schmidt, 1989), to provide stuof their health; they need relevant, appropriate, acceptdents with the skills and attitudes that would foster in „ able, accountable health services, accessible to them them an ethos of life long learning. for at least two where they live (who, 1978). decades, some medical educators have been exploring and developing this methodology as a means of address2.3 the role of the university in transing the challenges of the information explosion and the formation of the health service deficiencies of traditional health personnel training, which foster an attitude of passive, examination driven south african policy makers and health personnel learning, which ceases upon graduation with the end of ; "are faced with the challenge of meeting the people's examinations (lipkin, 1989b). around the world, more needs by redistribution of resources; different models than twenty medical schools from diverse political and of service; and by new and relevant skills and technosocial systems, have participated in the common experilogies. to achieve these goals, awareness of and sensiment of implementing this approach, since it was pio' tivity to the diversity of cultures, and the needs of the neered at m c m a s t e r university (lipkin 1989b). ethnically rich south african population, are essential. nonetheless, contingent upon the paradoxical context it is at this level that the dynamic role of the university of the traditional assumption that teachers in tertiary in the transformation process becomes apparent. not institutions need no training in teaching skills and only should relevant skills be identified and taught; methodologies, the impact of such ideas on other disciappropriate, new technologies developed; and attitudes plines is only recently being felt in any significant way modified; but also new and appropriate models of servin south african universities. ice must be developed and validated. furthermore, the at the university of durban-westville (udw) changuniversity has a.critical role to play in the development ing policies, which are dynamically impacting on proof appropriate policies. without policy change, transgressive thinking, are producing a rigorous demand not formation at other levels remains superficial and inefonly for maximally effective teaching methodologies, but fective. supported and informed by appropriate realso for a radical review of curricula. the university has search, which is a major goal of a university, transforcommitted itself to a creative, social redress programme mation, through development of appropriate models and to address the inequity of the past, in relation to stupolicies, becomes a viable reality, dent selection policies. in order to reflect a representative ethnic distribution across the student population, 2.4 background to the project it has been necessary to abandon traditional entry criteria, based solely upon matriculation results, to enit was within this framework that the intersure that educationally disadvantaged people have acdisciplinary health group at the university of durbancess to tertiary education. academics are thus chalwestville (udw), together with concerned academics lenged to develop effective and relevant teaching and from other health related tertiary academic institutions learning strategies. in the region, established the natal institute of community health education (niche). this is a tripartite 2:2 health service background partnership between communities, service organisations and tertiary academic institutions, which is primarily in south africa it is not only educationally that the aimed at changing the training of health personnel, majority of people have been disadvantaged, but it is socialisation is a critical part of health personnel edualso in terms of basic needs and health care. it has cation (friedman, 1991; lipkin, 1989b; schmidt, 1989). been acknowledged that the who goal of health for all the aims, therefore, are to expose the providers of the by 2000, is beyond reach (csd/swo report, 1992). service to appropriate models of care, and acceptable health and health care, therefore, are central issues in role models at a stage early enough to ensure the critical demands of the present climate in the counsocialisation to appropriate models of service. particitry. over the past decades, health care in south africa, pants collaborate in an interdisciplinary, community has reflected the principles of the biomedical model, in based, problem based, integrated teaching, research and which health is perceived as the absence of disease service initiative, in which the teaching methodologies (capra, 1983). this has resulted in a hospital and clinic are participatory, self directed student centred learnbased, curative service delivery system. the interacing with an emphasis on process, and skill development, tion of this system with the reality concerning inequirather than on content based, lecture centred learning, table resource allocation that has favoured urban fa a second, smaller initiative, specific to udw, is an cilities (medical research council, 1991), has led to a interdisciplinary research project,, designed to investisituation where vast numbers of rural people have had gate the implications of components of the same comlittle or no access to health care. the only possible opmunity based initiative; to use the findings to impact tion for developing a strategy to pursue the goal of upon curriculum and institutional transformation in the y the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) community based education in speech pathology ί university of durban-westville in an under served faculty of health sciences and health related departments. the feasibility study that is presented here, was located in the context of the above. in 1992, in the department of speech and hearing therapy, it was not possible to alter the curriculum in any significant manner, but in acknowledging the principles and aims described, the staff agreed that the need for more appropriate educational experiences was urgent, and relevant to present students as well as to future groups. it was considered to be of critical importance to evaluate the feasibility of implementing such a programme. the current timetable and the speech pathology curriculum were adapted, therefore, to allow for the introduction of an integrated, community based component that would, as far as possible, reflect the principles of both phc and an emancipatory (grundy, 1987), student centred teaching strategy. one day a week during the academic year (i.e., 20% of the final year curriculum), the final year class and a lecturer spent the day in a peri-rural community of approximately 67 000 people at kwa dedangendlale in the valley of a thousand hills, 40 kilometres from durban. it is situated in kwa zulu-natal, on the eastern seaboard of south africa. the black population is zulu speaking and mainly poor. it is served by a socio-medical phc project, the valley trust, established in 1951. so great are the needs of disadvantaged communities in south africa, however, that even in the context of a highly successful phc project such as this, the reality is that basic needs are still inadequately met, and that there is virtually no paramedical service. 3 description of the programme the programme comprised a number of components from both the theoretical and practical courses which were co-ordinated to provide a cohesive programme. i 3.1 purpose of the programme j there were two major goals of the programme: 1) to investigate the feasibility of decentralised community based student centred learning, that reflects the aims of niche (described above), by reviewing the extent to which academic standards can be maintained | ii) to sensitise students to, and to facilitate the development of appropriate skills for community based service, by exposure to third world conditions. 3.2 components of the programme 1) research design module. the programme commenced with a ten hour course on practical aspects of designing and implementing a research project. evaluation of this module was based upon the written report of research projects undertaken by the students. 2) seminar module. in the 20 hour seminar module, students presented topics1 that had been selected to develop familiarity with the literature; to stimulate their own thinking; and to provide them with a relevant theoretical background. the starting point of this module was a global, first world profile of the profession of audiology at the mmunity 95 speech, language pathology and audiology (slp&a). the purpose of this was to identify paradigms and international, current trends, including phc, that would provide them with a framework for analyzing, applying and understanding professional issues when operating in third world conditions; and to facilitate understanding from the outset, that the implementation of phc principles in no way implies a lowering of standards nor a "second class" service. other topics related to theoretical aspects of social issues such as poverty; to discipline specific and methodological issues; and to the implementation of phc. the purpose of this module was to allow the students to identify within themselves, insights and resources that they would be bringing to the programme from their three years' experience of a traditional first world, eurocentric curriculum; and to obtain an initial recognition of the limits and inadequacies of such a curriculum for the conditions they were about to experience. 3) field visits. upon completion of the seminar module, visits to kwa dedangendlale commenced. the phc manager of the service organisation of the valley trust project, a medical doctor with an epidemiological background and vast phc experience, worked closely with the departmental lecturer and the students throughout the programme. as students from many disciplines visit this area, the programme was planned to involve as many disciplines as possible, e.g. nursing, medicine, occupational therapy. students from other disciplines moved in and out of the programme for short periods of either one or six weeks: these were involved as much as possible in the programme to allow students of different disciplines the opportunity of working together on common projects; to learn to understand each other; and to recognise common difficulties and constraints. the objectives were few, and simply formulated to provide maximum flexibility that would allow students and staff to develop the programme iii response to the needs of the community, and to negotiate the course of the programme in collaboration with them. through experiential, participatory, reflective learning methods, appropriate insights and skills were targeted. both small group and plenary workshops were utilised for preparing students who could not speak zulu, for communicating with the zulu speaking community people; for the development of research projects; and for planning intervention and service. contacts took place at schools; in the local court house, in a first aid station and in people's homes. 3.3 the field visit programme 1) orientation. in order to ensure that the community people were protected as much as possible from blunders by inexperienced students, the orientation was focused upon the following : the community, through interview and focus groups with community health workers; teachers; and with key members of the valley trust staff • living conditions and the work of community health workers through home visits with them • the socio-medical project through input from the valdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 96 ley trust staff • methodological and cultural issues relating to cross cultural, people centred, facilitative communication through workshops and role play.' 2) input concerning slp&a. contact with the chws and the teachers commenced by discussion concerning their conceptualisation of the discipline and the services available: people were asked to provide input concerning their understanding of slp&a • students then interacted with them to provide more accurate information. 3) identification of community needs. using the shared framework concerning the nature of the discipline of slp&a, community members were asked to identify their needs relative to this. (as there is a long established socio medical project working on basic needs, these were not the focus of the current programme.) 4) negotiation of a research topic. based upon the identification of needs, students were required to negotiate a research topic, with either the chws or the teachers, that would be of relevance in some way, in addressing an expressed need. as the students' final year research project was integrated into the community practicum, it was required to be of benefit to the community. it was therefore required to include an action phase that would provide a service to the community, thus integrating the goals of research and service with those of teaching. they were permitted to develop topics of their own interest, on condition that they were agreed by either the teachers or the chws. they were encouraged, however, to choose the simplest topics possible, due to the challenging complexities of the task, viz.: that an entire class was required to run their projects in a particular community; that they would therefore be obliged to be working in the context of a language not known to them; and that there were no available precedents of linking the research project to teaching and service in this way. 5) preparation for visits. workshops were used to enhance cultural sensitivity; cross cultural, cross linguistic communication; and facilitative methods to ensure dialogue and participation. methods used were experiential and participatory learning; brainstorming; role play; and reflective review techniques. 6) planning. all aspects of the programme were carefully planned by both staff members and the students. i) the ten research proposals (appendix 1) were discussed in a group, so that the students were exposed to, and participated actively in the development of all the projects, thus expanding their practical research experience. the planning for these was detailed, and included discussion of relevance, accountability, acceptability, participation and action, as well as logistical factors, and academic and design issues. ii) group data collection was used in order to maximise the research experience for the students and the the south glen w. jager chws; and to use the research experience for providing hands-on community work. for this, the projects were grouped together in themes. very careful planning, therefore, was critical to achieve this in the time available. three days were targeted across three weeks, but due to unforeseen difficulties data collection was extended to five days. iii) after individual analysis of the data, action programmes were designed to be of some benefit to the community. although these focused to a large extent on the transfer of information and skills, identified needs of individuals were met as far as possible. iv) implementation of the intervention programmes was planned together, by the students. this was a major challenge because of constraints in terms of available resources. 7) staff and student responsibilities. initially the staff took a directive role, taking responsibility for introducing the students to relevant concepts and issues: they initiated and modelled the methods and techniques used. supervisory and monitoring functions became their primary responsibilities as the programme progressed. the students gradually assumed the responsibility for directing the planning and implementation of the programme after the first six weeks. their responsibilities involved identifying and effectively utilising the minimal human and service resources available; identifying human contact chains to overcome the lack of the telephone service; time management; and meeting the needs of all parties in so far as possible, in a relevant, appropriate manner. 4 evaluation in an integrated, problem based, experiential teaching initiative such as the above, traditional student evaluation methods are inappropriate. as this was primarily a feasibility study, implying the need for programme evaluation, it was critical that the evaluation procedure should be sensitive to the nature of the experience. this was discussed by staff and students,j both informally and in a workshop session. a procedure that would reflect as far as possible, all aspects of the discussion, was then designed by the departmental lectjurer. this was presented to and accepted by the students and the valley trust staff member. it was agreed that the feasibility of the study would be determined to a great extent by the evaluation of the students: both by tiheir own self-evaluation of the initiative as a learning experience, and by external evaluation of their performance. it was therefore necfessary to determine the students' subjective perceptions ofboth the experience and their performance; and to identify indicators of performance that could be used in a more objective evaluation protocol. 4.1 the evaluation procedure 1) significance of the experience to students. as the students believed strongly that their experience should be shared, they agreed as a group, that the programme should be documented and published. they agreed to write personal descriptions of the significance of the experience to them (appendix 3), and they mancan journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) community based education in speech pathology and audiology at the university of durban-westville in an under served community 97 dated the departmental lecturer to write the paper. the need for open, frank disclosures was stressed, but the students indicated that this would not be a problem, as the nature of the experience had made them feel quite comfortable about this. the descriptions were written without consultation, in order to identify individual, personal perceptions. it was left to them to identify themselves or not as preferred. 2) evaluation ef students'participation and input to the project. as the students had been responsible as a group for a large part of the activities of the programme, it was agreed that it would be necessary to obtain an evaluation of the contribution made by each student, on dimensions of responsibility (list 3) and leadership (list 4) to his/her own project (list 1); to the other students' projects and to the programme as a whole (list 2). this was done by means of a rating scale (1-5) with categories agreed by the students and staff (appendix 2). both staff members also rated each student. 3) questionnaire: student's evaluation of the programme. anonymously, students completed detailed questionnaires that provided information concerning the nature of their perceptions, evaluations and recommendations; and concerning the strength of their opinions. they were requested to provide as much negative information as possible, as this would be the most valuable part of the exercise for developing the programme. 4) interviews: assessment of student performance. in lieu of a traditional, formal final assessment of performance within the context of the placement, the departmental lecturer interviewed each student for approximately twenty minutes, at the end of which a negotiated percentage mark was given. the following format was used. , i) initial presentation by students. the students were asked in advance to review the experience and to identify significant issuesjand indicators of insight and performance. they were asked to present whatever they believed would demonstrate their insight and ability; and to evaluate the experience, their input, their development and their performance. i ii) students' recommendations. recommendations and motivations for improving the programme were sought, to provide another dimension to the evaluation procedure and to provide input into the development of the programme. iii) marking overall performance. students then provided and motivated a percentage mark. at this point i assigned (but did not disclose), a mark based upon my observations and evaluation of the student's role and performance in relation to the entire process; the general trend of his/her profile as indicated on the class rating matrix; and based upon the student's performance in the above interview. after the student's motivation, i facilitated discussion to substantiate and to probe further, to allow the student to reformulate or reiterate the evaluation. at this point, both the student and i reflected further on our marks, to permit us to modify them, after which the student discussed his/ her final mark. i then gave input concerning my mark and my evaluation, after which a final mark was agreed. 5) research projects : academic evaluation of written research report. students' performance on these projects would be a particularly clear indicator of success. although they were required to be accountable; to have an action phase; and to be participatory if at all possible, they would nonetheless, have to meet required academic standards. this aspect was measured by the usual practice of evaluation by two departmental internal examiners not immediately involved in the programme, and an external examiner from a university with a strong academic focus. 6) final examination: academic evaluation of performance in course major. departmental examinations in speech pathology in the final year reflect the full course of four years of study, with three, three-hour written papers and a 30 minute clinical interview with evaluation by a panel of all the departmental academic and clinical staff and an external examiner. a similar examination schedule applies to audiology, but as this component of the curriculum was not altered it will not be discussed. 7) career: programme impact on choice. the medical education literature (e.g., schmidt, 1989) suggests that one reliable indicator of success of a problem based, community based approach to education, is the chosen career path of the student after graduation. students whose attitudes have been positively altered by conscientisation and whose skills for third world models of service delivery have developed, are more likely to choose phc types of posts than those who have not received positive benefits from the exposure. in order to tap this source of information therefore, students were asked before and after exposure to the programme to provide information about their preferred career choices, given that all things would be equal. 4.2 evaluation results 1) the written descriptions were all extremely positive. the only negative comments related to initial reservations and anxieties. 2) the ratings providing information concerning students' perceptions to the programme indicated diverse levels of evaluation. seven students (n=13) used a narrow rating band, at the higher end of the scale, for all students for responsibility for own projects. five used this high, narrow rating band for contribution to total project; four for responsibility for total project; and three for leadership role for total project. two of the students consistently used this narrow band on all four lists, and two did so on three lists. others made use of the full range of ratings, all of which reflected the ratings of the two staff members involved, indicating consensus and the ability of students to evaluate their own and other's work. 3) the questionnaires were extremely positive in favour of the experience, in that no responses fell below die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 98 glen w. jager the second highest category, and most were at the highest level. such strong positive results raise the question that students might not have felt free to provide negative information. this, however, was not the case, as the students provided detailed critiques as well as detailed motivations for their opinions. in the former, all students provided clearly negative comments, together with constructive recommendations for modification. 4) the student interview corroborated and developed the conclusions reached, concerning the questionnaire information. although some students lower in the range tended to reflect inadequate insight, which led to an inflated mark, and some appeared to underestimate their performance, the general pattern was one of maturity and insight. the negotiated marks ranged from 53% to 78% with a median of 67% and a mean of 66.5%. only two of the students were led to lower their marks and three were led to raise them. for the others, which differed from mine by only two or three points, a compromise was agreed halfway between the two marks. 5) the research projects were passed by all students, and the departmental staff received a congratulatory letter on the success of the programme from the external examiner. the marks ranged from 54% to 76% with a median of 64.4% and a mean of 64%. 6) in the final examinations all students except one, passed. the unsuccessful student was one who had consistent difficulties, and one of two students who had received student ratings of making little contribution to the total project and of some reliance on the efforts of others for own project. the external examiner commented favourably on the quality of the answers on the examination paper that specifically related to, and reflected the principles of the programme. 7) career choices differed markedly before and after the programme, with changes from 9 to 2 for hospital based posts, and from 1 to 3 for community clinics, in favour of a shift from 0 to 6 for community based posts (figure 1). these reflect the students' comments that they felt able to deal with the challenges of community based experience and that they perceived the need for and significance of providing services at this level of care. 5 discussion both the major goals of the programme were achieved. a clear answer was obtained to the question of feasibility of decentralised student learning. academic standards are imminently maintainable as evidenced by the 100% pass rate on the research projects; by the sensitivity and range of the negotiated marks which reflected evaluation of performance levels on other, more traditional evaluation procedures; and by indications derived from the comments of the external examiner concerning a pattern of positive qualitative differences in the component of the written examination based upon the programme. the other goal of sensitising students by exposure to third world conditions and of facilitating the development of appropriate skills was also clearly achieved as reflected by evaluations by students and staff and by the changes of attitudes and self-confidence reflected in the shifts in career choices. the reality of the students' competence was affirmed by subsequent clinical events, such as a highly successful screening initiative in a school in an entirely different, first world context. for the first time in the history of the department, the organisation and planning of such an initiative was left entirely to the students: the role of the staff being one of observation and monitoring only. the screening proceeded smoothly and efficiently while the students remained calm and competent throughout. clearly they had been able to extrapolate and apply the lessons of the valley programme to the new context, thus substantiating their comments on the value of the experience not only to service delivery in a phc model, but also to first world service delivery. from the programme in kwa dedangendlale it was also possible to identify the potential role of speech, language pathology and audiology in a phc model of service delivery. although in the limited exposure we could do little more than touch upon such issues, it was very clearly possible to work in a language and culture different from our own, and to have a place in phc service delivery. we were not able to develop relevant models of service delivery. this will be a continuous process that must take place over a number of years. there were strong indications, however, of the importance of an interdisciplinary context and of developing skills and delivery models appropriate to, and relevant, to service needs. the benefit to the community of an ad hoc service was indicative of the future role that could be played by speech, language and hearing therapists in such a context. although the programme reported was little more than a feasibility study, its impact on those involved was significant. the community people we worked with have reported that they have gained both knowledge hospital h h ^ ^ ^ ™ ™ ^ 2 special school j ^ ® community clinic h ^ s community based s l h t gggj g e n e r a l p h c η 0 2 4 6 8 / 1 0 12 number o f s t u d e n t s ^ aftnr before figure 1. student career choice before and after programme the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) community based education in speech pathology and audiology at the university of durban-westville in an under served community 9 9 and some skill relating to a broader range of health problems; but most of all they have rated as most significant, their role in teaching future health professionals about the reality of life and the lack of health care in their community. they too are aware of the inadequacies and points of failure, but they are convinced of the value of the programme, and are committed to working towards strengthening it. the ownership of the user community for this programme has grown, so that more and more, they are directing the programme by the ever increasing clarity of the articulation of their needs and expectations. people in neighbouring communities are urging us to expand our activities to them. students who have now graduated, continue to seek forums for the presentation of the initiative. they are convinced of their role in sharing their experience and their insights, in contributing to transformation, and in recruiting colleagues. they believe they should continue to work for a service that will meet the needs of all people; and to agitate for relevant changes in the infrastructure of the health system. academics are committed to developing the programme and involving all disciplines, despite the enormous challenges of collaborating with and working with so many academics and professionals who have been accustomed to working independently. 5 .1 lessons learnt it has been possible to identify factors that could be likely to hinder and promote the greatly needed process of curriculum transformation within the university. such factors are thus critical in the process of strengthening the role that the university can play in meeting the needs of the community it serves. 1) the challenge. it became very clear that the nature of community work, that is based upon the principles of the who definition of comprehensive phc, is both slow moving and demanding. integral components of participation and development imply dimensions that are new to many health personnel and academics, whose training was based on the mechanistic paradigm of the medical model (capra 1993). the contingent service delivery model demands anj urgency for action directed towards a curative outcome for individuals. the phc approach, on the other hand, demands a more equal, facilitative, participatory model of service, based on the principles of identification of needs and resources and on the provision of options. inherent in this approach is the development of capacity to facilitate the active participation of the "client". the challenges of re-orientation, conscientisation and skill development in both students and staff, result in anxiety and insecurity, as they venture out of the safety of the hospital and the clinic to risk (a) failing to meet the academic and educational needs of the students and (b) exposing disadvantaged community people to inexperienced students. 2) the need for time. one lesson that emerged from the programme in 1992 and that has been reinforced subsequently, is that the process is slow initially. while students continue to experience a sense of impatience, there must be sufficient time to plan, to develop skills, to reflect and negotiate. in this time, while the relationship with the community is being developed, and the direction of the programme is being negotiated to meet the needs of all concerned, time must be spent on developing students' skills. they need to learn, inter alia, how to communicate in a language most of them do not speak, with people from a different culture, who are mainly uneducated.2 they must learn to negotiate, not inform; they must learn to abandon professional jargon; they must learn to facilitate client participation, not act "upon" their clients. all this takes time, humility, sensitivity and respect; and while students continue to feel a sense of impatience, they need to continue to prepare and plan, for they have not yet learnt the necessary lessons and skills! 3) the potential for success. another critical lesson from the experience is the reassurance that if undertaken carefully, this period of skill development, planning, and negotiation, is richly rewarded. suddenly the process speeds up and the anxiety gives way to a sense of the richness of the experience, which continues to grow throughout the year. in the feasibility study, inadvertently, inadequate time was scheduled for planning in the action phase. although impatient and anxious initially, the students themselves recognised the inadequacy of the first contact during the action phase, and immediately requested additional time from the staff members, as they acknowledged that the need for careful planning was as great as in the early preparation phase. this time, however, although severely constrained by time, there was no impatience and certainly no frustration. instead a note of c a u t i o n a r y admonishment towards the staff could be detected. 4) factors hindering and promoting the transformatory programme. promotive factors identified are the following: the clear indications that academic standards could be maintained while adding new dimensions of accountability, relevance and capacity development • the strong positive conviction concerning the feasibility and value of the programme which replaced initial scepticism, anxiety and frustration. hindering factors identified are the following: lack of human resources, which impacts upon all other hindering factors, presents as the most significant single obstacle lack of adequate, thorough preparation and planning, action concerning goals, methods, co-ordination, development of skills and utilisation of resources • attempts to hasten the process and to take short cuts time tabling difficulties that prevented common starting and ending dates for students from different departments 5.2 reflections on the role of the university in disadvantaged communities after initiating an integrated interdisciplinary programme in a disadvantaged community, it appears that the university's role could realistically be to impact upon die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 100 glen w. jager change directly within the community. by the sharing of skills and resources and by strengthening the initiatives of the community, it is possible for the university to contribute directly to development. indirectly too, by transforming the curriculum for educating health personnel, the university can facilitate the development of appropriately trained health personnel who, in the long term, will contribute to transformation of the health system and health service delivery in the country by their sensitivity and accountability to all the people of the country. in south africa, however, the role that the university can play is even more significant than this, for it can, and should be at the vanguard of profound change. it is not enough to provide a community service, even one that is enabling; nor is it enough to alter the attitudes and skills of health personnel and other professionals of the future. changes in health personnel education must be parallelled by policy change. not one of the students who would prefer a community based post has been able to find one. the infrastructure of the health services have remained unchanged. the threats of further privatisation and elitism continue to flourish. the significance of the university's role in health, therefore, is not only the development and validation of appropriate models of service delivery, but the development and implementation of policy informed by relevant accountable research. it is only through informed policy change and the development of appropriate models of service that profound transformation can occur. it is imperative therefore, that the university has a comprehensive, well co-ordinated, integrated programme of teaching research and service that will inform policy change and implementation, to address the challenge of development, with a strong focus on a meaningful programme of social redress. parallelling the inequities in the health service, are the inequities in education which continue to deny university access to the historically disadvantaged people of the country. it is not enough that the disadvantaged communities are served by sensitised health personnel from other communities: they are rightly demanding that their own people have easy access to university education and that they are served by adequately educated health personnel form their own communities. for this to become a reality, universities must be proactive in impacting upon education of disadvantaged communities at every possible level, from pre-school to adult education. to do this, every aspect of life must be addressed, including socio-economic conditions. the university with the richness of a multiplicity of departments, is well placed to lead in sensitisation to the need, and in addressing the challenges. by adopting and implementing the principles of the multi-sectoral approach to phc, the university will become accountable to the community it serves, and integral to the process of transformation and so will meet its social and moral obligations of contributing to development of the communities it serves. although education cannot transform the world, the world cannot be transformed without education. prof. j. reddy. vice chancellor and rector, university of durban-westville (1991). installation address. 6 conclusion in this paper, an integrated teaching, research and service programme has been described. the aims of the study which were to provide alternative skills and to sensitise students to third world conditions; and to investigate the feasibility of final year speech, language, hearing therapy students working in a disadvantaged community with people of a different culture, in a language not known to the students, were both achieved. by linking an academic module and a research commitment to a clinical service practicum, it was possible to provide an experiential learning component, introducing an alternative model of service delivery of relevance, not only to people living in third world conditions. a qualitative analysis of the programme indicated that lack of resources and lack of co-ordinated and adequate planning could seriously compromise such a programme, which is challenged by the need for profound attitude change. potentially promotive factors relate to the indicators of success which suggest that academic standards are imminently maintainable; and to the strong positive impact on those involved. reflections relating to the role of the university focused on the development of appropriate models of health care and policy in transforming not only the health service, but also education and socio-economic conditions of community life. the multi-sectoral perspective of phc was cited as providing the integrative cohesion that will render the university accountable to the community and integral to the processes of transformation and development. notes 1 the list of topics, materials used and other information are available from the author. 2 according to the government census of 1985, 50% of south africans over the age of 20, and 79% in rural areas, are functionally illiterate. (medical research council, 1991, p.18.) ι acknowledgements | ι the c o n t r i b u t i o n made by the people of kwa dedangendlale, the community health workers, thei students, members of the staffs of the valley trust and the department of speech and hearing therapy are warmly appreciated. in exploring new models of learning jand service provision, we have attempted to establish relationships in which participants all assume the roles of both teaching and learning. in such a context, although i take responsibility for the opinions expressed, i acknowledge the invaluable contribution of all concerned, to a profoundly enriching experience and i thank them all for contributing so much to our common learning. i acknowledge all those who have commented on the draft of this paper, and particularly the former students who have shared the experience. their comments on the paper have been particularly helpful. although the feasibility study reported was not funded, the subsequent pilot programme is part of broader initiatives, described in the paper, which are being funded by the w κ kellogg foundation, battle creek, michigan and the international development research centre, ottawa, canada, respectively. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) community based education in speech pathology and audiology at the university of durban-westville in an under served community 101 correspondence should be addressed to: glen jager, niche academic co-ordinator, c/o idhig f2-416, university of durban-westville, private bag x54001, durban 4000. phone (031) 820-2905 fax (031) 820-2925 references bruner, j. (1966). toward a theory of instruction. new york: norten. capra, f. (1983). the turning point: science, society and the rising culture. london: collins. csd/swo report (1992, october). "health for all" out of reach. csd/swo bulletin, 3-4. fraser, c. (1931). the case study method of instruction. new york: mcgraw-hill. friedman, i. (1991, november). the education of health personnel for primary health care: current practise and prospects for the future in south africa. paper presented at the symposium: training of health personnel, medical research council, cape town. grundy, s. (1987). curriculum: product or praxis. lewes, east sussex: falmer. katz, f.m. & fulop, t. (eds.). (1978). personnel for health care. geneva: who. lipkin, m. (1989a). toward the education of doctors who care for the needs of the people: innovative approaches in medical education. in h.g. schmidt, m. lipkin, m.w. de vries, & j.m. greep (eds.), new directions for medical education: problem-based learning and community-oriented medical education. new york: springer-verlag. lipkin, m. (1989b). preface. in f.m. katz & t. fulop (eds.). personnel for health care. geneva: who. medical research council. (1991). changing health in south africa: towards new perspectives in research. menlo park, ca: h.j. kaiser family foundation. rogers, c. (1969). freedom to learn. columbus, oh: merrill. sached trust. (1985). right to learn. durban: sached. schmidt, h.g., de vries, m.w. & greep, j.m. (eds.). (1989a). new directions for medical education: problem based learning and community oriented medical education. new york: springer-verlag. schmidt, h.g. (1989). how effective are problem-based, community-oriented curricula: experienced evidence. in h.g. schmidt, m.w. de vries & j.m. greep (eds.), new directions for medical education: problem based learning and community oriented medical education. new york: springer-verlag. walsh, w.j. (1978). the mcmaster programme of medical education, hamilton, ontario, canada: developing problem solving abilities. in h.g. schmidt, m. lipkin, m.w. de vries, & j.m. greep (eds.), new directions for medical education: problem-based learning and community oriented medical education. new york: springer-verlag. world health organisation. (1978). report on the international conference on primary health care at almaata, ussr, 6-12 september. geneva: who. appendix 1: research projects appendix 2 : student rating information 1 screening for loss and middle ear disease among black, rural infants and pre-nursery children in the ,4-36 months age group. | 2 feasibility of an alternative hearing screening protocol for pre-schoolers inl a black rural community. 3 hearing screening of rural black schoolchildren 6-14 years old in the inkazimulo lower primary school, at the valley of a thousand hills. 4 prevalence of hearing impairment in black class i underachievers in inkazimulo lower primary school. 5 communication disorders: beliefs and practices in a zulu community. 6 teacher attitudes and knowledge of stuttering held by primary school and high school teachers in the kwa nyuswa community. 7 acquired neurogenic communication disorders service need identification using community health workers in the kwa nyuswa area. 8 prevalence of hearing impairments and the feasibility of selected hearing screening tools for the elderly, black zulu speaking population of nyuswa. 9 the applicability of the draw a man test as used by speech-language therapists, to black children in a rural area. 10 the use of communicative intents by a group of black south african preschool children in the valley of a thousand hills. class evaluation of individual students' contribution to the valley trust experience instructions: 1 on the attached form, against each student's name, including your own, give your rating using the scale given below, to signify your perception of the following: list 1: responsibility taken for own project list 2: contribution made to total project list 3: responsibility taken for total project list 4: leadership role taken for total project 2 please do not identify yourself in any way 3 please complete independently and honestly notes to be completed by one student only • each student will get a separate form i will collate all responses on to separate lists i.e., 1-4, so please ensure accuracy scale: 1 = minimal mainly depends on others 2 = little frequently depends on others 3 = meaningful but does not undertake extra 4 = significant input is above average 5 = outstanding input is outstanding die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 102 glen w. jager appendix 3 : examples of students' descriptions 1 "the man who has never had to face adversity has yet to learn what he is made of' (anon). i believed that each day at campus was a day of facing adversity; of rising to the challenge and meeting the stresses of speech and hearing therapy. i guess i still believe this; however, from a changed perspective. when i'm on the brink of a nervous breakdown i just cast my mind back to my experiences in the valley of a thousand hills. this is written more from a personal level than an academic level, since it was my personal self that was so deeply affected by what i've observed and learnt over the year. to hear about poverty, to read about poverty has no real impact until you stare it straight in the face. the ongoing, painful adversity these people have to face leaves one totally shaken. this has been the first shocking awakening i've experienced in my short life a life previously so safe and cocooned! reality struck and left its mark. i've taken so many things for granted my three hot meals a day, a switch that will give me light, a tap that provides water people in the valley of a thousand hills are deprived of these basic needs needs which i believe are a right not a privilege. i have learnt such valuable lessons over these few months. for example, when we visited one home, walking over one hill to the next, the rugged terrain would leave me out of breath and dying for a thirst quencher and then i saw a young girl of approximately 11 years carrying a crate of cold drinks on her head 12 heavy bottles! she also had to walk over those hills! i've had to learn about mothers and fathers unemployed trying desperately to provide for their numerous children... however, a striking feature was the hospitality and friendliness of these people to us strangers, together with their sense of pride! there was no sign of (rightly placed) bitterness or hostility! another important lesson! i had no idea of the extent of poverty and its effects! there are so many other ignorant people out there people who may have the power to help in their own little way! people need to become aware of the poor socio-economic situation in these rural communities! awareness can lead to action! one way of action is through primary health care a topic i've become quite familiar with thanks to my experiences in the valley of a thousand hills, where i learnt about the dedicated work of the community health workers; and also thanks to valley trust and dr. irwin friedman for broadening my horizons! this year we were pioneers in conducting research in the valley! it was a starting point! it has also made me realize that may be through our profession we'd be able to ease the burden of these people! if only a little. i've also learnt of the importance of multi-disciplinary work there is no way we can work in isolation and be effective there has to be input from all sides! in addition, we as professionals/academics have to get rid of this "big chip on the shoulder" called arrogance! as paulo freire said "transformation is only valid if it is carried out with the people, not for them..." in essence, from: martin luther king: i have the audacity to believe that people everywhere can have three meals a day for their bodies, education and culture for their minds, and dignity, equality and freedom for their spirits. i believe that what self-centred men have torn down, other-centred people can build up. i still believe that one day humanity will bow before the altars of god and be termed triumphant over war and bloodshed and nonviolent redemptive goodwill proclaim the rule of the land and the lion and the lamb shall lie down together and every man shall sit under his own vine, and fig tree, and none shall be afraid. i still believe that we shall overcome. nobel peace prize acceptance speech. if the ignorant are educated, if the powerful act, if those who are status-bound lose their arrogance... maybe we will overcome. 2 the valley trust experience... whew!... how do i begin to describe it?! a roller-coaster ride: where i learnt about life, values, prejudice, politics, morals and about myself up every hill, down every dip and around every hairpin bend on the route. i've learnt about community: the concept; and phc what that term does and does not compass; i've acquired to a certain extent cultural sensitivity and tolerance of others' values, life styles and practices. i've learnt about the essence and value of communication, and being an effective communicator. ι most importantly i've been forced to "see" what i've been "looking at" ... a great many people in south africa have been deprived of essential services. a vast amount of work needs to be done. it is extremely encouraging to witness the community projects that a!re operating in the valley of a thousand hills knowing that each small step that is taken here, is a great step for the rest of south africa. ' thank you for the experience! the feeling has been incredible! research: "it was never easy, but together we made it!" the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 37 the manifestation of middle ear pathology in an elderly group ν j cilliers, β log (pretoria) a van der merwe, d phil (pretoria) μ hurter, β log (pretoria) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of pretoria ο nel, mb chb (pretoria) department of otorhinolaryngology, university of pretoria abstract there is a lack of clarity in the literature regarding the manifestation of the structural changes due to aging in the middle ear and the pathology which occurs in the elderly. in order to determine the incidence and manifestation of middle ear problems in an elderly group, acoustic immittance measurements and otoscopy were carried out on 94 subjects over the age of 65 years. thirty eight percent of all the subjects tested had abnormal tympanometry results in one or both ears. these abnormal results were due to either pathologies which were medically diagnosed, or other unidentified factors such as possible structural changes in the middle ear as a result of increased age. otoscopy and acoustic immittance measures should always be carried out as part of the test battery for the elderly. opsomming in die literatuur is daar geen duidelikheid oor die manifestasie van strukturele veranderinge in die middeloor weens veroudering en middeloorpatologie by die geriatriese bevolking nie. ten einde die insidensie en manifestasie van middeloorprobleme by 'n geriatriese groep na te gaan, is akoestiese immittansiemetings en otoskopiese ondersoeke uitgevoer op 94 proefpersone oor die ouderdom van 65 jaar. agt en dertig persent van al die proefpersone het abnormale timpanometriese resultate in een of beide ore vertoon. die abnormale resultate was die gevolg van verskillende middeloorpatologiee wat gediagnoseer is asook onge'identifiseerde faktore soos moontlike strukturele veranderinge in die middeloor weens veroudering. otoskopiese ondersoeke en akoestiese immittansiemetings behoort deel uit te maak van die toetsbattery vir geriatriese persone. many physical and behavioural changes differentiate the elderly population from younger persons (wofford, 1981). these physical changes include structural changes, specifically in the middle ear, such as degeneration of muscles and the stiffening of ossicular joints. there is no consensus in the literature as to the significance of these structural changes. such changes could have an effect on the functioning jof the middle ear and should be detectable through acoustic immittance measurements. there is also a lack of clarity in the literature as to whether these structural changes] lead to an increase in middle ear pathology (chermack, 1981). some studies have examined acoustic immittance measurements in the elderly. blood and greenburg (1977) examined the static acoustic immittance measurements in persons between the ages of 50 and 70. they found a significant decrease in values and concluded that there was a need for a different set of norms to be used when testing people over 70 years of age. nerbonne, bliss and schow (1978) investigated the static acoustic immittance measurements in subjects between the ages of 20 and 79 years. they found a slight but non-significant tendency for values to decrease with age. they recommended, however, that further study is neccessary in subjects over 79 years of age. degeneration of middle ear muscles results in an increase in acoustic reflex thresholds (chermack, 1981). however, studies by gelfand and piper (1981) indicated that there was no difference between the acoustic reflex thresholds of an elderly population with normal hearing and that of a young population with normal hearing. these studies have only investigated single aspects such as static immittance measurements or acoustic reflexes. the optimal use of acoustic immittance measurement is achieved when all three tympanometric parameters are assessed, viz. tympanometric peak pressure, tympanometric shape, as well as static immittance (margolis and shanks, 1985). it is also still uncertain whether there is an increase in middle ear pathology due to the structural changes. turner (1982), states that middle ear pathology is common among older patients. he found perforations of the tympanic membrane, otitis media with effusion, usually resulting from upper respiratory infection of influenza and otosclerosis. otitis media often occurs because of eustachian tube dysfunction (meyerhoff and paparella, 1978). eustachian tube dysfunction in the elderly is frequently a result of the degeneration of the veli palatine muscles (chermack, 1981). the literature indicates therefore that middle ear pathology does in fact occur in the elderly. there is no indication of the incidence of the pathology or whether the incidence is significant. these shortcomings in the literature have important implications for the audiologist. firstly, if these structural changes in the middle ear have a significant effect on immittance measurement, a different set of acoustic impedance (immittance) norms would be necessary for the elderly. secondly, if these structural changes lead to an increase in pathology, the audiologist should identify potential disdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 © sash 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 orders for referral, as many of these problems can be remedied medically or surgically (turner, 1982). thirdly, a significant incidence of middle ear disorders can motivate the inclusion of acoustic immittance measurement in the test battery for the elderly. the need for further study in this field is evident. the goals of this study are, firstly, to determine how structural changes in the middle ear affect acoustic immittance measurement. this evaluation will include tympanometric peak pressure, tympanometric shape, static immittance and acoustic reflexes. secondly, this study will examine the incidence of middle ear pathology within the test group (cilliers, 1987). methodology goals the goals of the study are: — to determine the types of tympanograms which occurred most frequently in a specific group of elderly subjects. this can supply information regarding the influence of age on the tympanogram. — to relate the acoustic immittance results to the diagnosis of pathology in order to determine whether the results are influenced by pathology or other factors such as structural changes due to aging. — to determine what effect aging has on the static immittance values and, if this varies significantly from the prescribed norms, a different set of norms for the elderly should be considered. — to record the presence or absence of acoustic reflex measures in the group of elderly subjects. experimental design a one-group design was used for this study. the same group of subjects was subjected to the test battery and each subject underwent the same procedure. subjects criteria for selection — age: the subjects had to consist of persons over the age of 65 years as sixty-five was accepted for medical-legal purposes as the start of "old age". many physiological [changes occur in this age group. these include structural changes of the auditory system (wofford, 1981). — health: all the subjects had to be capable of undergoing acoustic immittance testing and an otoscopic examination. i /' i — sex: the subjects could be either male or female. this is essential as some research claims that there is a difference between static immittance values of males and females (jerger, jerger and mauldin, 1976). therefore • both male and female subjects were selected for this study. selection of subjects ninety-four people from three old age homes in pretoria were randomly selected for this study. the ages of persons in the experimental group varied between 66 and 95 years of age. there were 11 male subjects and 83 female subjects. ν j cilliers, a van der merwe, μ hurter and ο nel the age spread and sex of the subjects are presented in table 1. table 1: the age-spread and sex of subjects used for this study age of subjects no of subjects sex of subjects male female 65—69 years 5 0 5 7 0 7 4 6 0 6 7 5 7 9 27 2 25 8 0 8 4 34 5 29 8 5 8 9 14 3 11 90 + 8 1 7 apparatus acoustic immittance data were obtained on each subject with a calibrated (to standard -iso 389, 1979) grason stadler gsi 28a auto tymp, utilizing a 226 hz probe tone. acoustic reflex measures were obtained at 500, 1000, 2000 and 4000 hz. experimental procedure each subject tested underwent the following procedure: a short interview was carried out to obtain personal information, viz. name and date of birth. a preliminary visual examination of the external auditory canal had to be carried out to determine whether immittance measurement could be obtained. immittance measurements cannot be carried out if fluid is running from the ear canal or if there is an obstruction (meyer, hurter and van rensburg, 1987). the acoustic immittance protocol was then carried out. the probe was placed in the ear canal and an airtight seal was obtained. the test sequence proceeded automatically. the pressure sweep began at + 200 dapa and proceeded in a negative direction until it reached -400 dapa. the ear canal volume in ml, immittance peak in ml, and the pressure peak in dapa were recorded (margolis and shanks, 1985). acoustic reflex measures were then obtained ipsilaterally at 500, 1000, 2000 and 4000 hz in db hl (hearing level), i i i an otoscopic examination was then carried out by a clinical assistant in otorhinolaryngology in order to determine the presence of any middle ear pathology. / data analysis since all the acoustic immittance measures were carried out on the gsi 28a, the norms were used as set out in the instruction manual. — static immittance: the normal range is 0,3 ml to approximately 1,8 ml. a static immittance peak which falls between these two ranges indicates normal mobility of the middle ear system. — pressure peak: for most applications a normal pressure range of -150 dapa to +100 dapa is used (margolis and shanks, 1985). however, strict rules for a normal middle the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the manifestation of middle ear pathology in an elderly group 39 ear pressure indicate a pressure range of -50 dapa to + 50 dapa (brooks, 1981). — tympanometric shape: — type a tympanogram has static immittance values of between 0,3 ml and 1,8 ml and has a pressure peak at or near 0 dapa although a range between -150 dapa and + 100 dapa is considered normal (hodgson, 1980; margolis and shanks, 1985). — type ad signifies a tympanogram with an unusually high static immittance peak of 1,8 ml or more. peak pressure is usually at or near 0 dapa (hodgson, 1980; margolis and shanks, 1985). — type as denotes a tympanogram with reduced amplitude of 0,2 ml or less. the pressure peak is usually at or near 0 dapa (margolis and shanks, 1985). — type β tympanogram is flat, typified by the absence of a pressure peak (hodgson, 1980; margolis and shanks, 1985). — type c tympanogram has a negative tympanometric peak pressure, usually smaller than -150 dapa (hodgson, 1980, margolis and shanks, 19^5). — acoustic reflex measures: the reflex usually occurs between 70 and 90 db hl above the hearing threshold in normal;hearing people (wiley and block, 1985). data processing for the analysis of the data obtained in this study descriptive statistical techniques were used. the aim of descriptive statistics is to sum up and condense the measurable characteristics of results obtained. the following techniques were used: percentages and frequency tables. i resijlts j / / · i the results are presented in the sequence outlined according to the goals of this study. the types.of -tympanograms which occurred table 2 presents a summary of the salient data obtained iq this study. it shows the number of subjects with normal acoustic immittance results bilaterally, with abnormal acoustic immittance results unilaterally and with abnormal immittance results bilaterally. the types of tympanograms which occurred' are indicated as well as the number of ears which presented each type of tympanogram. table 2 also shows the various types of pathology which were diagnosed. of the 94 subjects tested 58 (62%) had type a tympanograms bilaterally. type a tympanograms are associated with normal middle ear function. this pattern reflects normal mobility and peak pressure (hodgson, 1980). twenty three of the subjects (24%) had abnormal results unilaterally and 13 subjects (14%) had abnormal results bilaterally. therefore 38% of all the subjects tested had abnormal acoustic immittaijce results in one or both ears. these results are extremely high, compared with a study by jerger (1976). jerger found that the highest incidence of abnormal tympanometric results (31%) occurred in the age group 2 to 5 years. it was suggested th t̂ tnis age group is the highest risk group for otitis media. jerger found that there was a gradual decrease in abnormal results with an increase in age. ) with reference to the number of ears tested in this study 188 ears were examined in total. of these 139 (74%) yielded type a tympanograms. of the remaining ears 38 (26%) presented type as tympanograms. type as denotes a tympanogram with a reduced amplitude characteristic of ossicular fixation, tympanosclerosis and some forms of otitis media (margolis and shanks, 1985). the type as tympanogram therefore predominates over the other abnormal tympanograms in this elderly group. this differs from findings in children where tympanograms associated with otitis media, eg. type β and type c tympanograms, occur most frequently (hodgson, 1980). the reason that the type as tympanograms occurred more often in the elderly group could be due to the calcification and ossification of the joints between the ossicles in the middle ear (kahane, 1981). there were 3 type β tympanograms. type β tympanograms occur in the presence of middle ear effusion and other spaceoccupying lesions of the middle ear. they can also occur in cases of tympanic membrane perforation and impacted cerumen (hodgson, 1980; margolis and shanks, 1985). most of these pathologies associated with type β tympanograms can be remedied medically or surgically and therefore can be identified and referred for further treatment (turner, 1982). there were 5 ears with type c tympanograms. this type of tympanogram is often an indication of eustachian tube dysfunction or otitis media (hodgson, 1980; margolis and shanks, 1985). of all the ears tested tympanograms with no peak were recorded in 3 ears. this could indicate a perforation or impacted cerumen (margolis and shanks, 1985). correlation between the acoustic immittance results and the types of pathology diagnosed the question as to why this high percentage of abnormal acoustic immittance results occurred was posed and thus the acoustic immittance results are discussed in terms of the different types of pathology which were identified. this is also presented in table 2. type a tympanograms of the 139 ears which yielded type a tympanograms 96 were diagnosed as having normal middle ear function. forty of the remaining ears with type a tympanograms were diagnosed as having cerumen, ear canal collapse or both. although cerumen and ear canal collapse can affect pure tone audiometric results (wofford, 1981), they do not necessarily affect acoustic immittance results (randolph and schow, 1983; wofford, 1981). this could explain why type a tympanograms occurred. a further 2 ears that presented a type a tympanogram were diagnosed as having healed perforations in the tympanic membrane. a mildly scarred tympanic membrane can result in increased mobility in the tympanic membrane (jerger, anthony, jerger and mauldin, 1976). in these 2 cases it appears as though the healed tympanic membrane had not necessarily affected the tympanometric results. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 . r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 ν j cilliers, a van der merwe, μ hurter and ο nel ό 7—1 7—1 7—1 τ—1 n or m al ο 00 l£> 7—1 co 7—1 oo n o. o f ea rs l£> τ—1 τ—1 co ν oo 7—1 τ—1 co 1 ο ν ν ν cm ty pe o f ty m pa no gr am ty pe a ty pe a ty pe a s ty pe β ty pe c n o pe ak ty pe a s ty pe β t yp ec n o pe ak n o. o f su bj ec ts oo in co ν on or m al a co us tic im m itt an ce re su lts bi la te ra lly a bn or m al ac ou st ic im m itt an ce re su lts un ila te ra lly a bn or m al ac ou st ic im m itt an ce re su lts bi la te ra lly die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the manifestation of middle ear pathology in an elderly group 41 one ear with a type a tympanogram was diagnosed as having otitis media. it should be noted that a peak pressure of -119 dapa was recorded. this could imply that the peak pressure norms that were used, viz. a pressure range of -150 to + 100 dapa, are not sensitive enough to identify all cases of otitis media. type as tympanograms thirty eight of the ears tested yielded a type as tympanogram. of these 21 were diagnosed as having normal middle ear function, 5 were diagnosed as having cerumen, 1 had impacted cerumen, 3 had ear canal collapses, 5 had both cerumen and ear canal collapses, 2 had tympanosclerosis and 1 ear had had surgery to the tympanic membrane. the percentage of ears with type as tympanograms that were diagnosed as normal is 55. this implies that factors other than pathology influenced the tympanometric results. structural changes due to aging such as stiffening of the tympanic membrane and ossicular joints could have resulted in the type as tympanogram (wofford, 1981). the possibility that structural changes due to aging may have affected the tympanometric results, has important implications for the audiologist, as there will be a high percentage of abnormal tympanograms with no evidence of middle ear disorder. this implies that the occurrence of a type as tympanogram does not always indicate the presence of middle ear pathology among the elderly. impacted cerumen can cause a type as tympanogram (margolis and shanks, 1985). this accounts for the single case of cerumen impaction which yielded as type as tympanogram. ear canal collapse and cerumen do not necessarily influence the acoustic immittance results (randolph and schow, 1983; wofford, 1981). there were 13 ears with type as tympanograms that were diagnosed as having cerumen, ear canal collapse or both. these tympanometric results could therefore be caused by factors other than pathology. structural changes due to aging can /cause stiffening of the middle ear system, which could result in / type as tympanograms. there were also 2 ears which had tympanosclerosis and 1 case of surgery to the tympanic membrane that yielded type as tympanograms. according to margolis and shanks (1985), both of these diagnoses could] result in a type as tympanogram. type β tympanograms there were 3 ears with type β tympanograms. one was diagnosed as having a perforated tympanic membrane and the third had a retracted tympanic membrane and cerumen. all three of these types of pathology correlate with the acoustic immittance results (margolis and shanks, 1985). referral for further medical attention is essential in these cases (turner, 1982). type c tympanograms of the 5 ears which yielded c tympanograms 2 had otitis media. the remaining 3 ears all had an ear canal collapse. an ear canal collapse cannot influence tympanometric results (wofford, 1981). the type c tympanogram could therefore be caused by eustachian tube dysfunction. tympanograms with no peak three ears with tympanograms which had no peak were diagnosed. perforated tympanic membranes were found in 2 of the ears. the third ear had cerumen and an ear canal collapse. no peak is registered in tympanometric measurement when the pressure that is required for the measurement of the middle ear function cannot be built up (hodgson, 1980). both the perforations and the ear canal collapse could result in no pressure being built up. there is therefore a correlation between the tympanometric results and the presence of pathology. the effect of aging on static immittance values in order to determine the effect of aging on the static immittance values a correlation was drawn between the two. table 3 gives the mean and median static immittance values as a function of age. table 3: correlation between age and mean and median static immittance results in millilitres (mp) age in years mean median 6 5 6 9 0,61 mp 0,65 mp 7 0 7 4 0,75 mp 0,65 mp 7 5 7 9 0,53 mp 0,4 mp 8 0 8 4 0,58 mp 0,4 mp 8 5 8 9 0,38 mp 0,4 mp 90 + 0,55 mp 0,5 mp the results show that there is a significant decrease in both the mean and median static immittance values in the subjects of 75 years and older. this decrease does not, however, continue with an increase in age. there is in fact, a slight increase in the static immittance values of subjects over 90 years of age. this tendency contrasts with the findings by blood and greenburg (1977). they found a significant decrease in static immittance values. their study examined subjects between the ages of 50 and 70 years. the fact that their population was considerably younger than the population used in this study may explain the resulting difference in findings. the tendency for static immittance values to decrease slightly and then level off as was observed in this study, correlates with findings by nerbonne, et al. (1978). they found a slight but non-significant tendency for values to decrease with age. their subjects ranged in age from 20 to 79 years. they did recommend that further study of individuals over 79 years of age was necessary. this study examined subjects between the ages of 65 and 95 years, but the results did not differ from those found by nerbonne, et al. (1978). a separate set of static immittance norms for the elderly is not necessary, as the mean and median values for all the age groups fall within the normal range of 0,3 mp — 1,8 mp. acoustic reflex measures table 4 presents the percentage of subjects as well as the percentage of ears with absent acoustic reflex measures. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 ν j cilliers, a van der merwe, μ hurter and ο nel table 4: percentage of subjects and ears with absent and present acoustic reflexes percentage of ears percentage of subjects acoustic reflexes present 48% 29% acoustic reflexes absent 52% 71% the acoustic reflex is usually elicited at 70—90 db hl above pure tone threshold. the most likely cause of the absent reflexes is an increased pure tone threshold. most elderly people have a sensory neural hearing loss due to presbycusis. since pure tone audiometric testing was not carried out, the exact cause of the absent acoustic reflexes is unknown. the possibility exists that the absent reflexes could also be the result of the degeneration of the middle ear muscles. all the muscles in the body degenerate with increasing age. the muscles in the middle ear should not be an exception. discussion of results the manifestations of outer and middle ear pathology outer and middle ear pathology occurred in 38% of all the ears tested. this high occurrence can partially be explained as a function of increased age. the type of pathology which occurred most frequently was excessive cerumen which may obstruct the external auditory canal partially or completely (cohn, 1981). copious secretion of wax occurs in individuals of all ages; marshall (1985), however, indicates that excessive cerumen is more common among older people. an overaccumulation of cerumen can result in a conductive hearing loss and abnormal acoustic immittance results, depending on the degree of obstruction (wofford, 1981). excessive cerumen can also influence ear mould impressions for hearing aids. twenty two percent of all the ears tested were diagnosed as having cerumen, and of these, 64% had type a tympanograms. the acoustic immittance results indicate that even a total occlusion of the ear canal due to cerumen impaction does not always result in abnormal tympanograms. the impaction may, however, influence the results, and this implies that an otoscopic examination should always be carried out prior to acoustic immittance testing in an elderly group. excessive cerumen can be treated quickly and successfully. ear canal collapse or stenosis occurred in 17% of all the ears which were examined. although ear canal collapse can occur in any age group, it is usually associated with old age which causes a loss of elasticity in the dermis. the cartilaginous portion also becomes more flexible (wofford, 1981). the external auditory, canal closes when an earphone is placed over it; this can result in a mild to moderate conductive hearing loss (randolph and schow, 1983). a type a tympanogram can occur with reduced ear canal volume measurements. this correlates with the findings of this study where 64% of the ears diagnosed as manifesting an ear canal collapse had type a tympanograms. the remaining 36% had additional factors which affected the results. in order to prevent incorrect interpretation of audiometric testing, otoscopy and tympanometry should always be performed prior to audiometric testing. a collapse of an ear canal during audiometric testing can be prevented by using circuaural or postaural cushions (marshall and grossman, 1982). there were 3 cases of otitis media diagnosed and 1 case of retracted tympanic membrane, indicative of an early stage of otitis media (cohn, 1981). the development of otitis media is most frequently related to eustachian tube dysfunction. the eustachian tube maintains middle ear ventilation and facilitates the clearing of foreign material as well as providing immunological defence (cohn, 1981). eustachian tube dysfunction is often associated with old age because the veli palatini muscles which open the eustachian tube may degenerate or atrophy. kahane (1981), says that degeneration of all the muscles occurs with old age. otitis media may therefore be caused indirectly by increased age. however, it is also possible that these subjects have a history of otitis media, and thus the condition is not as a result of old age. otitis media cannot always be detected through pure tone audiometry. it can yield a conductive hearing loss. otoscopy and acoustic immittance testing can detect the presence of otitis media. as mentioned previously, a type β or c tympanogram usually occurs (hodgson, 1980) and this correlates with the tympanometric results obtained in this study. it is therefore essential that these examinations be carried out as part of the test battery for the elderly. otitis media is therefore not exclusively associated with the very young, but can also occur among the elderly. there were 3 ears with perforated tympanic membranes. tympanic membrane perforation can result from excessive effusion, the erosive effect of middle ear lesions such as cholesteatomas or an external trauma (wofford, 1981). in the 3 cases diagnosed in this study, the most likely cause was excessive effusion. a perforation may affect the audiometric results, depending on the size of the perforation (wofford, 1981). a perforation can be identified through otoscopy and acoustic immittance measurement. if the 3 cases identified in this study had perforations due to excessive effusion, the possibility exists that the otitis media was caused, by a eustachian tube dysfunction. the eustachian tube dysfunction, as mentioned earlier, could be the result of increased age. tympanosclerosis and healed perforations occurred in 4 ears. tympanosclerosis and healed perforations may result after tympanic membrane rupture and healing (wofford, 1981). tympanosclerosis can include stiffening of the tympanic membrane, tympanic mucosa, ossicular ligaments1, tendons of the stapedius and tensor tympani muscles and fixing the malleus andincus in the epitympanic area. a scar·! red tympanic membrane is indicative of a healed perfora-; tion. the most likfely cause of a healed perforation is effusive otitis media. j it is essential that the persdn with evidence of a healed per-j foration undergoes regular ,'otoscopic examinations and1 acoustic immittance testing to detect the recurrence of otitis 1 media. / / it is evident; therefore, that much of the pathology which occurs in' elderly individuals could be as a result of increased age. otoscopy and acoustic immittance testing are essential for the diagnosis of most of these disorders and must therefore be carried out prior to pure tone audiometric testing. repeated testing is necessary to detect the recurrence of otitis media. the nursing staff who work in old age homes could also be trained to identify symptoms related to outer and middle ear disorders for early identification and referral. the effect of structural changes due to aging on, the acoustic immittance measurements eleven percent of abnormal acoustic immittance results the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the m i f e s t a t i o n of middle ear pathology in an elderly group were diagnosed as having no middle ear disorders. this implies that there are factors other than a middle ear pathology which resulted in the abnormal acoustic immittance results. one of the possible causes of these abnormal results could be attributed to structural changes to the middle ear system. kahane (1981), noted that the structure of the adult laryngeal cartilages changes with increasing age. calcification or ossification caused stiffening of the laryngeal joints. these changes do not only occur in the larynx, but occur in all the joints of the body including those in the middle ear. it seems possible, therefore, that a stiffening of the middle ear system could cause abnormal acoustic immittance results. fifty five percent of the type as tympanograms which occurred had a diagnosis of normal middle ear condition. abnormal results could also have been caused by a history of pathological conditions which are no longer evident, but which have caused a permanent effect on the middle ear system. since biological changes are known to occur with increasing age, it is unlikely that the middle ear should remain unaffected. these non-pathological changes which influence acoustic immittance measures have important implications for the audiologist. there will thus be a percentage of abnormal tympanograms with no middle ear disorders in the testing of the elderly. although this percentage is not high enough to warrant separate static immittance or tympanometric norms for the elderly, it nevertheless should be taken into consideration. > it is also of interest that the static immittance values decreased in subjects over 75 years of age. this decrease did not continue with an increase in age. in addition, these values increased slightly in the age group over 90 years. the mean and median static immittance values of each age group fell within the normal range, i.e. 0,3 ml — 1,8 ml. conclusion \ in this group of elderly subjects, acoustic immittance results indicating a normal middle ear system (type a tympanogram) occurred most frequently. of these normal acoustic immittance results, 43 earsjwere, however, medically diagnosed as having excessive cerumen, ear canal collapse, healed perforations or otitis media. these diagnoses do not necessarily influence acoustic immittance measurements. thirty-eight percent of the subjects had abnormal tympanograms in one or both ears. of these tympanograms indicating middle ear dysfunction; eight ears presented with type β or c tympanograms or had no peak, confirming the medical diagnosis of conditions such as otitis media. two of the ears had type c tympanograms but there was otoscopically no indication of middle ear pathology. these results could indicate a eustachian tube dysfunction possibly caused by degeneration of the veli-palatini muscles. of the abnormal tympanograms, type as, indicating reduced amplitude, occurred most often. this was otoscopically verified in 45% of the cases where a diagnosis of pathology such as tympanosclerosis or impacted cerumen occurred. however, 55% of these type as tympanograms had no evidence of middle ear disorder. these results could therefore be due to unidentified causes such as non-pathological changes, associated with old age, which lead to a stiffening of the middle ear system. the fact that there was a correlation between the decrease in static immittance values and an increase in age up to 90 years of age, seems to indicate that aging had an effect on the results. otoscopy and acoustic immittance measures should be carried out as part of the test battery for the elderly. identification of pathology, which may influence further audiometric testing, eg, ear canal collapse, is essential as the effect of these disorders on audiometric testing can be prevented. middle ear pathology increases the total hearing loss in the elderly. there is a high incidence of sensory neural hearing loss in the aged. the conductive component of the hearing loss can be remedied medically or surgically. the audiologist therefore has an important role to play in the identification ot these conductive components. the last few decades have seen a rapid increase in the number of people over 65 years. the elderly can no longer be ignored, but must be recognised for the contribution they make to society. there is a shift in the orientation of the field of audiology towards the evaluation and rehabilitation of the elderly. the audiologist plays an important role in the identification and referral of individuals who have pathological conditions in the outer and middle ear. references blood, i. & greenburg, h. acoustic admittance of the ear in the geriatric person. journal of the american audiological society, 2, 185—187, 1977. brooks, d. impedance measurement in the elderly. in r. penha & p. de ν pizarro (eds.) proceedings of the fourth international symposium in acoustic impedance measurement. lisbon: lissabon universidade, nova de lisboa, 1981. chermack, g. handbook of clinical audiological rehabilitation. illinois: charles c thomas, 1981. cilliers, n.j. the manifestation of middle ear pathology in an elderly group. unpublished b.(log.) dissertation, university of pretoria, 1987. cohn, a. etiology and pathology of disorders affecting hearing. in f.n. martin (ed.) medical audiology disorders of hearing. new jersey: prentice-hall, inc., 1981. gelfand, s. & piper, n. acoustic reflex thresholds in young and elderly subjects with normal hearing. journal of the acoustical society of america, 69, 2 9 5 2 9 7 , 1981. hodgson, w.r. basic audiological evaluation. baltimore: williams and wilken co., 1980. jerger, j.f. clinical experience with impedance audiometry. in j.l. northern (ed.) selected readings in impedance audiometry. new york: american electromedics corporation, 1976. jerger, j.f., anthony, l., jerger, s. & mauldin, l. studies in impedance audiometry iii. middle ear disorders. in j.l. northern (ed.) selected readings in impedance audiometry. new york: american electromedics corporation, 1976. jerger, j.f., jerger, s. & mauldin, l. studies in impedance audiometry i. normal and sensori-neural ears. in j.l. northern (ed.) selected readings in impedance audiometry. new york: american electromedics corporation, 1976. kahane, j.c. anatomic and physiological changes in the aging peripheral speech mechanism. in d.s. beasley and g.a. davis (eds.). aging communication processes and disorders. new york: grune & stratton, 1981. ' / margolis, r.h. & shanks, j.e. tympanometry. in j. katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology. baltimore: williaps and wilken co., 1985. marshall, l. audiological assessment of older adults. seminars in hearing, 6, 2, 1 6 1 1 7 5 , 1985. marshall, l. & grossman, m.a. management of ear canal collapse. archives of otolaryngology, 108, 3 5 7 3 6 1 , 1982. meyer, s., hurter, m. & van rensburg, f. gehoorsiftingsresultate van 'n groep kleurlingleerlinge. the south african journal of communication disorders, 34, 43—47, 1987. meyerhoff, w.l. & paparella, w.m. diagnosing the cause of hearing loss. geriatrics, 33, 95—98, 1978. nerbonne, m., bliss, a. & schow, r.l. acoustic impedance values in the elderly. journal of the american audiotory society, 4, 5 7 5 9 , 1978. randolph, l.j. & schow, r.l. threshold inaccuracies in an elderly clinical population: ear canal collapse as a possible cause. journal of speech and hearing research. 26, 54—58, 1983. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 turner, j.s. treatment of hearing loss, ear pain and tinnitus in older patients. geriatrics, 37, 1 0 7 1 1 8 , 1982. wiley, t.l. & block, m.g. overview and basic principles of acoustic immittance measurements. in j. katz (ed.) handbook of ν j cilliers, a van der merwe, μ hurter and ο nel ι clinical audiology. baltimore: williams & wilkens co., 1985. wofford, m. audiological evaluation and management of hearing disorders. in f.n. martin (ed.) medical audiology disorders of hearing. new jersey: prentice-hall inc., 1981. talking to professionals i'hc needier westdene organisation p.o. box 28975 sandrinaham 2131 telephone (oil) 485-1302/3/4/5 technimark 560 the south a frican journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abr audiometry: application to the paediatric population susan helfand ba (sp & η th) (witwatersrand) rachel yifat ma (sp path) (tel aviv) department speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract this study examines the relationship between auditory brain stem, behavioural observation and impedance audiometry in a paediatric population. particular reference is made to the strengths and weaknesses of auditory brainstem audiometry applied to this age group. results indicate that in some cases diagnosis of hearing loss is made primarily on the basis of this test, while in other cases the test serves as an excellent check on behavioural and impedance audiometry. opsomming die verhouding tussen ouditiewe breinstamoudiometrie, gedragswaarneming en impedans-oudiometrie is in 'n pediatriese groep ondersoek. verwysing word gemaak na die voordele en nadele van breinstamoudiometrie vir hierdie ouderdomsgroep. resultate dui daarop dat vir sommige pasiente die diagnose van 'n gehoorverlies uitsluitlik berus op die uitslag van hierdie toets, terwyl dit in ander gevalle dien as 'n aanduiding van die betroubaarheid van gedragswaarneming en impedans-oudiometrie. the impact of abr audiometry has increased rapidly over the past five years (hecox & jacobson, 1984). many more centres have introduced auditory nerve and brainstem evoked responses in addition to traditional behavioural procedures as a means of diagnosing hearing loss in infants and children (levi et al, 1983). initial validation procedures for auditory brainstem response (abr)* audiometry i.e. the degree to which the auditory brainstem potentials are able to accurately predict auditory impairment, were undertaken in the adult population (hecox & jacobson, 1984). finitzo-hieber (1982) cites her research, where impairments predicted by abr were confirmed by pure tone audiometric results, stating that, the agreement between abr and pta was generally good with abr/pta discrepancies among subjects never greater than one category. in other words, while abr could and did predict normal hearing in the presence of a mild loss, this discrepancy/error did not occur for moderate or greater impairment and a loss was never called severe to profound that was in fact normal or even mild in degree (sohmer & feinmesser, 1974 and pratt & sohmer 1978 cited by levi et al, 1983). hecox and jacobson (1984) also, using the pta as the validating reference criterion, report serious discrepancies in less than 1% of cooperative adults. therefore, whilst the use of click stimuli in routine abr testing does not allow for the prediction of information concerning the entire audiometric configuration, a methodological limitation generally accepted, one would usually still be able to identify patients in need of long term follow up (hecox & jacobson, 1984). recent studies have attended specifically to the use of abr testing in the difficult-to-test population, namely the very young and multihandicapped. cornacchia et al, (1982), comparing brainstem evoked response audiometry and behavioural audiometry in 270 infants and children (divided into those with and without reliable audiograms), found a higher agreement between the two methods in the former group but concluded that abr (using the term bsera) * suggested nomenclature at the international conference on standards for auditory brainstem response testing, cited by. sohmer, h. (1984). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 audiometry was reliable and enabled many diagnostic mistakes to be avoided. levi et al., (1983) conducting a similar, but two part study where both infants and neonates were studied, state that abr audiometry gave an earlier indication of the presence or absence of hearing and of hearing threshold. they recommended the use of abr as early as possible, particularly for the "high risk" infant. the purpose of this work was to analyze in more detail our own paediatric abr clinical data. specifically the aims were to examine and gain a better understanding of the relationships between abr, behavioural and impedance testing in this population group. method subjects during the period 1982-1983, forty-five children under the age of two years, were tested at the speech and hearing clinic, university of the witwatersrand. two years was taken as the arbitrary age cut-off point for the purpose of the study. fourteen children in this group were assessed with abr audiometry in addition to the conventional procedures and constitute the population under study. the children were referred by a variety of medical and educational sources for suspected or queried hearing loss. since abr testing was not administered routinely to all clinic patients in this age range, the majority of the children were those with associated problems or "at risk" infants. see table 1 below for a detailed description of the subjects (ss). as table 1 illustrates, eight of the ss had definite associated problems or "at risk" factors for hearing loss. in the remaining ss no "at risk" factors could be identified. procedure retrospective analyses of assessment reports took note of the results of three procedures: behavioural, impedance and abr audiometry. 1. behavioural observation audiometry (boa) all ss had been tested behaviourally in a sound field setting using © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 susan helfand and rachel yifat table 1 description of subjects subjects sex age associated problems at risk factors 1 μ 6 weeks — family history 2 f 5 months — 3 μ 5 months — 4 μ 7 months — 5 μ 8 months — 6 f 10 months downs syndrome 7 μ 16 months — 8 μ 18 months — 9 f 20 months pierre robin syndrome 10 f 20 months mental retardation 11 f 20 months reyes syndrome 12 μ 22 months cerebral palsy 13 f 22 months post meningitis 14 μ 24 months family history the methods reported by downs and sterrit (1967) cited and summarized by levi et al., (1983). essentially this is based on the use of higher intensity stimuli for younger infants: neonates 90-100 db hl; 6 week-old infants 50db hl (to which a gross motor reaction is expected); 4-5 months 30db hl; and 7-9+ months 10-20db hl (to which the expected response is a turning of the head to the source of sound). responses were categorized according to the following general descriptive categories: absent responses, queried responses and expected responses. in some cases a more detailed description of the expected responses was possible in terms of the intensity range. 2. impedance audiometry (imp) where available these results were described according to tympanometric configuration and acoustic reflex results. tympanograms were classified according to jerger's classification (jerger, 1970 cited by northern, 1984). reflexes were described as being absent or present at normal or elevated hearing levels. 3. abr audiometry (abr) abr testing had been conducted at both the national institute of personnel research and at the speech and hearing clinic, university of the witwatersrand. the test was carried out in an electrically shielded, sound-attenuated room. all children were sedated and slept through the entire test. the responses were elicited by clicks of rarefaction polarity with an electrical duration of 100 micro sees, delivered through a tdh-39 earphone monaurally. stimulus rate was 11.1/sec. electrodes were placed as follows: vertex (ev = active electrode); earlobe (er = reference electrode); and forehead (eg = ground electrode). the electrical signals were fed into a pre-amplifier, filtered through a band-pass,filter (150-3000hz), amplified by a lownoise wide band amplifier and then averaged. averaging was initiated at stimulus onset and continued for 10 ms. the averaged signals were printed on an x-y plotter. the procedure for threshold estimation was as follows: stimulus intensity was decreased in 5-10 db steps until no identifiable peak v was obtained. for the purpose of this study only the presence of peak v was used to estimate thresholds (chiappa, 1983). the site of impairment was not identified as the results for each individual were not compared to a normal latency intensity curve (hecox and jacobson, 1984). results results are discussed as follows: — agreement between boa and abr audiometry — agreement between impedance and abr audiometry — agreement among all three tests: boa, impedance and abr audiometry. agreement between boa and abr as can be seen in table 2 these infants (with the exception of subjects 10 & 12) had either normal or severe to profoundly impaired hearing, with no associated problems. this observation is consistent with the findings of gornacchia et al., (1982) who found in their group of children without associated problems (group a) that there was increasing agreement between boa and abr in those classified as profoundly deaf or no/mild hearing loss. specifically they noted that when audiograms revealed a hearing loss of at least 70 db in the frequencies of the speech range, the correlation between these two tests was perfect. ss 10 & 12 reflect how gross threshold 'estimates' (i.e. notjeompletely frequency specific) from behavioural testing were more or less in agreement with the extent of loss suggested by abr jaudiometry, despite the many queried responses. agreement between behavioural and abr test results should however take into account frequency specific responses. < | i as can be seen in table 3 agreements between abr.and boa in these subjects is generally poor. as in the case of group β ss in the study by cornacchia et al., (1982) these children were those with associated brain damage and/or behavioural problems, which render behavioural test results less reliable, especially in cases with severe handicaps. specific test results in this group are examples of how critical the contribution of abr testing was for clarifying the peripheral auditory status. in subject 6 where severe retardation may have confounded any impression of auditory functioning, abr results ensured that the problem of a concomitant severe hearing handicap was recognized and followed up much earlier. / in subject 11, a child with multiple problems, normal results as reflected on abr testing excluded a hearing problem and she could thus be referred accordingly. while these examples illustrate the serious discrepancies that may occur between the two tests, subject 14 is a good example of partial discrepancy between the two tests. here, / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abr audiometry : application to the paediatric population table 2 results of boa and abr in subjects reflecting good inter-test agreement subjects age associated problems boa abr i 6 weeks expected responses normal bilateral 3 5 months no responses severe-prof, s/n loss bilateral 4 7 months expected responses normal bilateral 7 16 months responses to low freq. only (70db hl) severe-prof, s/n loss bilateral 8 18 months sat l:95db hl r:85db hl severe-prof, s/n loss bilateral 10 20 months queried retardation response to 40-65 db hl many responses queried mild/mod s/n loss bilateral 12 22 months cerebral palsy lx:response at 70db hl many queries. 2x:response at 50-60db hl lx:severe loss bilateral 2x:mod. mixed loss bilateral 13 22 months post meningitis response at 90db hl severe-prof, s/n loss bilateral sat = speech awareness threshold s/n = sensori-neural severe-prof. = severe-profound table 3 results of boa and / / abr in cases reflecting inter-test discrepancy. / subjects age associated problems boa abr 2 5 months . prematurity response to medium intensity. many responses queried. normal bilateral 5 8 months expected responses ' mild cond. loss bilateral 6 10 months downs syndrome queried responses severe/prof, s/n loss bilateral 9 20 months pierre robin syndrome queried responses mild cond. loss bilateral 11 20 months reyes syndrome all responses queried normal limits bilateral 14 24 months family history responses at 60db hl severe/prof, s/n loss bilateral cond. — conductive s/n = sensori-neural severe/prof. = severe-profound die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 susan helfand and rachel yifat table 4 results of impedance and abr audiometry subjects age associated problems imp abr 1 6 weeks — bilateral type a normal bilateral 2 5 months premature bilateral type a ars at normal hls normal bilateral 3 5 months — severe-profound loss bilateral 4 7 months — bilateral type a ars at normal hls normal bilateral 5 8 months — bilateral type a ars at normal hls mild loss bilateral 6 11 months down syndrome grommets severe-profound loss bilateral 7 17 months — grommets severe-profound loss bilateral 8 18 months — grommets severe-profound loss bilateral 9 21 months pierre robin syndrome bilateral type β mild loss bilateral 10 22 months query retardation grommets mild-moderate loss bilateral 11 24 months reyes syndrome bilateral type a ars at normal hls normal bilateral 12 22 months cerebral palsy 1 χ bilateral type β 2 χ grommets severe-profound loss bilateral moderate loss bilateral 13 20 months post meningitis profound loss bilateral 14 24 months family history type a bilateral ars present up to ikhz, absent from 2-4 khz severe-profound loss bilateral key: ar acoustic reflex hl = hearing level behavioural testing gave the impression of a less severe loss than that indicated on abr, and probably yielded information about low frequencies that is not available with the use of the 'broad-band' click stimulus which reflects high frequency fibre activity (hecox & jacobson, 1984). , agreement between abr and impedance audiometry robier et al., (1983) discuss the value of using precise acoustic impedance measures to rule out middle ear effusion before administering electro-physiological tests to reveal sensori-neural deafness. levi et al., (1983) include impedance together with abr audiometry as a check on behavioural observation audiometry. neither of these studies, however, specifically examine the relationship between acoustic reflex threshold and abr measures. a comparison of the results of these two tests revealed how they may complement each other. it was possible to observe, in subjects 2 and 11, how abr results were able to lend support to the suggested normal hearing observed on the acoustic reflex subtest. in these cases, the presence of the reflexes at normal hls was ambiguous in the light of the queried behavioural responses. furthermore, in subject 14, while absent abr tracings suggested a severe to profound loss, the pattern of reflex threshold results.obtained (absent in the higher but present in the lower frequencies), suggested some hearing in the lower frequencies. since it is known that correlations between abr prediction of hearing loss and audiometric thresholds suggest that abr best approximates hearing thresholds at high frequencies (2-4 khz), and poorly reflects the integrity of hearing at less than 500 hz (galambo and hecox 1978, jerger and mauldin, 1978, cited by hecox and jacobson, 1984), acoustic reflex the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abr audiometry : application to the paediatric population 69 measures in this case, gave us information about possible residual hearing, otherwise unavailable from isolated standard abr testing. agreement among abr, impedance and boa in general the findings of the results of these infants reflected a familiar phenomenon facing the audiologist, namely that inter-test results are not always in perfect agreement. it is clear that where agreement is perfect the diagnosis of the extent and type of loss can be made more definitely, and that as agreement lessens diagnoses are made more tentatively. extending this to abr audiometric interpretation the use of adjunctive audiological information is crucial if one is to use abr results to effectively diagnose and manage, in particular the paediatric and unco-operative patient (hecox & jacobson, 1984). subject 12 best illustrates how the integration of results from all three tests avoided the possibility of a critical diagnostic error. on initial assessment abr together with boa results gave the impression of a bilateral severe hearing loss, while impedance results indicated the presence of some conductive component. subsequent to the medical treatment of the middle ear condition, abr and boa results together reflected a mixed but predominantly mild-moderate rather than severe loss, which was managed accordingly. although this is an isolated case it illustrates effectively how the exclusive use of abr audiometry cannot accurately predict permanent hearing loss. this is an important consideration when dealing with the paediatric population where the incidence of middle ear pathology is at its highest. conclusions there is no end to the combinations of the types and extent of hearing losses that an audiologist may encounter. certainly the small size and age range of this sample does not allow finite conclusions about the relative importance of abr, behavioural and impedance testing in the overall audiological test battery. nevertheless, closer inspection of the results and relationships between these tests provides an appreciation of the strengths and weaknesses of the use of abr audiometry in the paediatric population. j assuming that the abr threshold is a reliable indicator of the hearing threshold of a subject (sohmer & feinmesser, 1974; pratt & sohmer, 1978 cited by levi at al., 1983) five of the subjects in the study were diagnosed on the basis of abr results alone. these subjects were those with associated brainjdamage and/or behavioural disorders which made them less amenable to behavioural testing, and those where the presence of gronimets prevented the use of objective impedance measurements. in'these two instances abr testing enhanced earlier diagnoses of the peripheral auditory status and thus appropriate habilitation measures. in the remaining nine subjects, particularly those with normal hearing or severe losses in the absence of associated problems, abr audiometry served as an excellent check on both behavioural and impedance test results. of particular interest was the fact that the acoustic reflex sub-test served as a useful complement to abr results, at times providing information about possible residual low frequency hearing, which is not available from standard abr testing. while the ultimate validation of these abr test results would depend on follow-up audiograms, as was included in the study by levi et al., (1983), these results suggest nevertheless that abr audiometry can be seen as a logical component of paediatric testing. acknowledgement the authors wish to thank brian mallinson, chief researcher, nipr, human sciences research council, who assisted with some of the abr audiometric testing. references chidppa, k.h. evoked potentials in clinical medicine. raven press, 1983. cornacchia, l., viglian, e. and arpini, a. comparison between brainstem evoked response audiometry and behavioural audiometry in 270 infants and children, audiology, 21, 359-363, 1982. finitzo-hieber, t. auditory brainstem response: its place in infant audiological evaluation, seminars in speech, language and hearing, 3 (1), 76-87, 1982. levi, h., tell, l., feinmesser, m., gafni, m. and sohmer, h. early detection of hearing loss in infants by auditory nerve and brian stem responses, audiology, 22, 181-188, 1983. hecox, k. and jacobson, j.t. auditory evoked potentials. in hearings disorders, second edition, northern, j.l. (ed.). little brown and company, 1984. northern, j.l. impedance audiometry. in hearing disorders, second edition, northern, j.l. (ed.). little brown and co., 1984. robier, α., lemaire, m.c., garreau, b., polyet, m.j. martineau, j., delvert, j.c. and reynaud, j. auditory brain stem responses and cortical auditory evoked potentials in difficult to test children, audiology, 22, 219-228, 1983. sohmer, h. nomenclature and technical considerations of the short latency evoked potential. in international conference on standards for auditory brainstem response testing, starr, a. rosenberg, c., don, m., hallowell davis (eds.) edizioni techniche, milan, italy, 1984. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acoustimed's ha-2000 dynamic real ear insertion gain analyser represents a giant leap into the future now you can do true free-field tests without background noise interference right in your consulting room. test dynamic hearing aid performance in-situ with wide band signals (or even recordings of real speech), analyze a.g.c. characteristics or distortion components. the actual measurement time can be less than one hundredth of a s " 327 bosman building, 99 eloff street, johannesburg 2000. south africa. p.o. box 9988, johannesburg 2000. south africa. tel: 011-337-2977 w r i t e t o d a y a n d l e a r n h o w e a s i l y y o u c a n g e t h a 2 0 0 0 w o r k i n g for y o u . ha-2000 is supplied read\ complete with a fully progi micro-computer system and software for insertion gain specification. is this the ultimate in-situ analyser? acoustimed (pty) ltd the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a therapy programme for pre-school language impaired children* linda narun, m.a. (sp. path.) (witwatersrand) department speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg sum mar y this study aimed to establish effective language programmes for pre-school language impaired children based on psycholinguistic principles. eight language-impaired children 6 males and 2 females between the ages of 3 and 6 years, were the subjects. language samples were transcribed and subjected to syntactic analysis; semantic aspects were also considered and programmes developed for each subject, based on the information obtained from the language analyses. a developmental.sequence was adopted as the basis for therapy a 'slot-method' was used in which language was taught as rule-operated behaviour principles of discrimination learning were adopted for teaching some aspects of grammar · auxiliary verbs are reported in detail as this was the most universal error and difficult to teach. the role of imitation in language learning and therapy is discussed. opsomming hierdie studie beoog om doeltreffende taalprogramme vir voorskool se taalgestremde kinders, gebaseer op psigolinguistiese beginsels, daar te stel. die proefpersone was 8 gestremde kinders, 6 seuns en 2 dogters, tussen die. ouderdomme van 3 en 5 jaar. laalmonsters is getranskribeer en sintakties ontleed; semantiese aspekte is ook in ag geneem. programme, gebaseer op die gegewens verkry van die taalontledings, is vir elke proefpersoon opgestel. 'n ontwikkelingsvolgorde is as grondslag vir terapie aanvaar. η „01eul metode is gebruikvir die aanleer van taal as reelbeheerde gedrag. beginsels van disknminasie-aanleer is gebruik om sekere aspekte van grammatika aan te leer. slegs hulpwerkwoorde word uitvoerig bespreek. foute in hulpwerkwoorde was die mees algemene verskynsel en die_moeilikste om vir die kind aan te leer. die rol van nabootsing in die aanleer van taal en in taalterapie word bespreek. in the early years of life, communication occurs primarily through verbal language. the knowledge of, and ability to handle, the mother tongue is of prime importance if any further linguistic codes are to be learnt. any difficulty with verbal language must consequently affect all other aspects of communication. we are becoming aware of increasing numbers of young children who exhibit difficulties in acquiring the basic symbolic communication system. because of the importance of early language learning, it is imperative that more effective methods of intervention be developed. while many studies have been conducted on language acquisition,1'4'8'1 2'1 9' comparatively few have con* this paper is based on an m.a. dissertation submitted to the department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg 1976. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 linda narun cerned themselves with the practical application of this data to the therapeutic situation. lee1 0 describes the situation thus: psycholinguists have not studied atypical language development to any great extent nor have they suggested how to approach clinical problems in language acquisition. the further step of making psycholinguistic information useful and applicable to clinics is going to have to be taken by clinicians themselves. the aim of the study was to devise effective therapy programmes based on current psycholinguistic theory. while psycholinguistic research has added much to therapy programmes, language acquisition is still not fully understood, and so advances may not be as great as the volume of literature would have one believe. rees17 describes several theoretical bases which might be used in deciding the sequence of language therapy. one of these is the developmental sequence basis, which was adopted for the present study. according to rees, there are two implications inherent in this approach. firstly, the disordered language must be evaluated against what is expected. secondly, the child's language goes through predictable stages. during therapy, the child should be taken through these stages, rather than getting him to imitate an adult model. this approach may incorporate syntactic and semantic development. the present study attempted to utilise both aspects, but emphasis was laid on syntactic development. while many aspects of the grammar were diagnosed and treated, this article will deal primarily with the auxiliary verb to be, and sentence types dependent on its correct usage. in addition, factors which appear to influence language learning will be discussed. ingram5 demonstrates that there is a point at which structural aspects can be taught. he states that the problems that language deviant children manifest with the verb to be, are related to performance rather than competence. he found that these children not only had difficulty in acquiring the form, but also in supplying it when required. however, the sequence of development was the same as for normal children, albeit at a slower rate. thus, in terms of therapy, data on normal acquisition can be used. confirming brown's4 findings on the acquisition of is ingram5 suggests first teaching the contractible copula, and then the uncontractible copula, the contractible verbal auxiliary, and lastly, the uncontractible verbal auxiliary. this order is also based on the relative frequency of occurrence of these verb forms. the importance of the auxiliary verb seems to lie in its necessity as a structural component, without which fully grammatical sentences are not possible. according to lee,1 0 the elaboration of the verb is one of the most difficult aspects of the english language, because of the introduction of the auxiliary verb. she states that each auxiliary carries a meaning which is superimposed on the meaning of the main verb. however, with regard to the progressive form, brown,4 makes it clear that its semantics is not clearly understood. ', thus, while errors involving the verb to be were viewed predominantly as syntactic errors, it was felt important to make them as meaningful as possible so that acquisition would be facilitated. journal of the south african speech and hearing association. vol. 23. december j 976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for language impaired children 5 the syntactic realisation of question and negative sentences, is dependent upon a knowledge of the auxiliary verb. the syntactic development of these sentence types has been described fully.9 however, the semantic development is understood less w e l l , although brown4 stresses that the development of the w/2-question is highly dependent on semantic and cognitive features. similarly, bloom1 criticised klima and bellugi-klima's9 study on the grounds that it only considered syntactic development. she has identified three semantic features of negation i.e. non-existance, rejection and denial. a detailed discussion of these features will not be undertaken here. when devising therapy programmes, however, semantic features must be taken into account. method eight children, two females and six males, between the ages of three and six years, diagnosed at the speech and hearing clinic, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, as having language learning problems, acted as the subjects in the study. all subjects had normal hearing. in addition, no subject demonstrated any degree of retardation or severe brain injury. detailed diagnostic procedures were undertaken with each subject and, from the information obtained, individual therapy programmes devised. these diagnostic procedures were repeated afregular intervals. any change required in the therapy plan was made, so that diagnosis was on-going throughout the time period allowed for therapy. spontaneous speech samples of each subject (s) were tape-recorded. language was elicited in an informal situation, where the experimenter (e) exposed ss to meaningful objects and pictures. immediately following assessment interviews, the tapes were transcribed, so that contextual information could be considered in doing the analysis. together with the sentence produced by the s, the sentence he was attempting was also noted. at times it was difficult to determine exactly what the s had intended to say. it was here that contextual information was utilised in deciding the semantic intention of the sentences. without this information utterances cannot be adequately analysed or interpreted.2'4 sentences were assessed using theories of transformational grammar as advocated by jacobs and rosenbaum.7 however, it was often necessary to compare the s's performance with developmental sentence types described by various authorities.1 0'1 2'1 3 therapy morehead and johnson15 have pointed out that most programmes in speech pathology use learning theory as a basis for instruction. within this type of approach, the case is presented with a model sentence, is asked to repeat it and given immediate reward. most speech and language therapy is structured for direct immediate experience and the unstable changes that occur with the use of this technique are well-known to therapists and generally referred to as "carry over problems.15 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en geoorheelkunde. vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 linda narun the approach utilised in this procedure hoped to avoid the problems inherent in a behaviourist model, by attempting to teach language as a system of rule-operated behaviour. regarding language in this framework, it is hypothesised that language deviant children do not extract the rules governing the grammar of the language, as opposed to the normal developing child who somehow learns the semantic restrictions of various linguistic categories. the aim of therapy, therefore, was to enable the ss to extract and abstract syntactic rules and generalise these rules to novel and creative situations. thus, the child was not reinforced for a repetition of a model sentence, but rather for being able to generate novel sentences utilising rules taught in therapy. in this way, a 'cognitive' rather than 'behaviourist' approach is used. following the initial diagnosis, a therapy programme was devised for each subject. therapy was conducted over a six month period, during which time each child was seen twice weekly. an attempt was made in devising the therapy programme to establish at what point during the acquisition of syntax, the child had made an incorrect hypothesis in the learning of syntactic rules. each programme was devised to stimulate the child at the point at which he failed to establish correct phrase structure rules and to enable him to 'releam' parts of the grammar which were dependent on the acquisition of these earlier rules. a 'slot method' was employed in an attempt to demonstrate the rule that the s had failed to acquire. it was felt that if imitation of a constant structure could be established (where the syntactic structures and semantic relations were held constant) the s might abstract the rule operating and generalise it to new creative utterances. thus the basic structure of the sentence was held constant and the lexical item of one category varied in order to demonstrate the rule which gave rise to the underlying meaning of the sentence. the subject was presented with many sentences illustrating one aspect of a sentence before any response was demanded. in utilising the slot method, stimulation was not only through the auditory modality, but through the visual modality as well. therefore, verbal stimuli were always associated with visual stimuli, arranged sequentially, in an attempt to heighten awareness of auditory sequencing of a sentence. in some instances it was difficult to apply the slot method utilising visual stimuli as described above. an example of this was in the teaching of the present progressive construction. when teaching this construction the therapist and the s carried out the continuous action in unison with the verbal counterpart. when acquiring the phonological system children learn the difference between phonemes rather than discreet features. this theoretical framework was utilised in teaching aspects of syntax. in the case of tenses, an attempt was made to demonstrate the difference between, for example, an action that was ongoing and one that had already taken place. therefore, the child carried out the activity "johnny is jumping" while he verbalised it, and afterwards said "johnny jumped", indicating that the activity had already occurred. alternatively, the future tense was used preceding the introduction of the present progressive. in this way, the different tenses were semantically and syntactically contrasted. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for language impaired children 7 data was evaluated qualitatively rather than subjected to a statistical analysis. the linguistic performance of language impaired children tends to be inconsistent. the nature of these inconsistencies is important and forms an integral part of making therapeutic decisions. in evaluating the efficacy of therapeutic procedures, initial and final language samples were compared. results the primary aim of this study was to investigate a method of therapy. however, the diagnostic procedures undertaken prior to the development of therapeutic programmes in themselves yielded interesting results. in addition, the therapy programmes depended on these results, so that the diagnostic findings will also be reported. the results confirm those of morehead and ingram,14 who found that a significant discriminating factor between language impaired and young normal children is the restricted ability of deviant children 'to develop and select grammatical and semantic features which allow existent and new major lexical categories to be assigned to larger sets of syntactic frames.' morehead and ingram see these restricted sentences and sentence types as being reflective of a cognitive deficit. an example of the restricted sentence types used by the ss in this study are vvft-questions. no subject used all possible wft-word questions. the only two types used by most subjects with any consistency were where and what. the study analysed many aspects of syntax, e.g. prepositions and prepositional phrases, pronouns, articles. however, as stated earlier, only auxiliaries will be discussed and to a lesser extent sentence types dependent upon their realisation. diagnostic results all ss demonstrated errors in the use of the auxiliary verb system. the use of the present progressive construction fee + ing confirmed previous observations by lee,10 in that the inflection -ing was never omitted, but the auxiliary was always omitted. according to her, in the course of normal development -ing emerges very early and is later followed by the auxiliary: the language,impaired child, therefore, seems to follow the normal course of verb acquisition, but in a typically delayed manner. brown4 in comparing adam and eve's performance on the auxiliary with labov's work, found that the normal child may omit auxiliaries (or copulas) which can be contracted but not those which cannot be contracted. however, on the rare occasions that the auxiliary was used by some ss, the findings of brown (on normal children) were not demonstrated. these language impaired children, only used contracted auxiliaries (and copulas). these isolated examples of the usage of the auxiliary, did not appear to reflect the child's competence to handle this node for two reasons: (i) the sentences in which the auxiliary appeared, seemed to be imitated forms i.e. stock sentences which the mother may often have used, e.g. i'm big, i'm good. all examples seemed to be of this type, (ii) within the samples, auxiliaries were used very infrequently and in restricted sentence types, i.e. declarative sentences almost exclusively. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gelioorheelkunde. vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 linda narun menyuk13 has pointed out that, although all developing behaviour is inconsistent, the normal child tends to use the correct construction with greater frequency than the incorrect construction. in contrast, the language impaired child in this study used the incorrect construction with far greater frequency. in addition, brown" has stressed that frequency must be assessed together with diversity of type. the results of this study demonstrate the very restricted occurrence of this verb. by attaching -ing to the verb, the child has marked the class, as well as the continuing action. the semantic loading of be is very low and the child communicates no more meaning by using it. as the child uses language to communicate his cognitions, he cannot be expected to use a linguistic form until he understand i t . 1 9 as brown" has pointed out, the young child using socalled 'telegraphic' speech can be understood very easily. including functors does not really add significantly to the child's message. however, the normal child very quickly incorporates functional words into his utterances, whereas the language impaired child fails to do so. auxiliaries are but one example of this failure. it seems possible that auxiliaries and their development are related to"the cognitive deficit postulated earlier. therefore, because this verb has so little meaning, therapy should attempt to make it more meaningful. t h e r a p y r e s u l t s the verb to be proved to be a very difficutl construction to teach and took a great deal of time. lee1 0 has suggested that more meaning can be brought to this verb by teaching questions and negatives first, where the necessity for the verb is more easily demonstrated. if this procedure had been adopted, therapy time required may have been significantly reduced. however, having taught it in this manner an interesting result was observed. once the ss could use this verb correctly, questions and negative transformations spontaneously improved. no s had to be given therapy specifically dealing with these two transformations. why this occurred, is difficult to explain. however, it confirms that the ss did not have difficulty with questioning or negation per se, but with the syntactic structure involved in the formulation of these sentence types. this does not, however, explain how the child was able to transpose the auxiliary and the subject by merely acquiring the auxiliary verb. errors involving the verb to be were the most frequent, in that this verb was deviant in all ss and only very infrequently was it used correctly. it was the base line of therapy with all ss, because of its universal nature and because so many other constructions were affected by it. at the end of the sixmonth period allowed for therapy, five of the eight ss handled this'node quite competently. the remaining three subjects experienced difficulty in generalising the auxiliary to new situations. after many repetitions of the construction, they were able to imitate it, but had difficulty in generating new utterances. it would seem that these subjects had difficulty in abstracting and generalising the rule governing auxiliaries. again this particular difficulty seems to indicate a cognitive deficit in these particular ss. ι general discussion the verb to be both as an auxiliary and a copula proved to be a difficult journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for language impaired children 9 construction to teach. lee1 0 suggests that because it has a low semantic value, its acquisition is difficult. it is the therapist's task to increase the meaning of this node if it is to be adequately taught. the method employed in this experimental procedure i.e. the use of contrastive tenses together with the appropriate actions did seem to highlight this verb. therefore, this broadly syntactic approach was successful in that the children incorporated the structure into their utterances. this led to the correct usage of many sentence types. after many repetitions of this construction, ss were able to imitate it. immediately following these repetitious presentations, the ss were able to produce it spontaneously. for those ss who encountered difficulty the problem arose when they used this construction at a later time, when not specifically stimulated. it was thus the 'carry-over' and 'generation' of sentences involving the present progressive construction which invariably proved to be the point of difficulty. an attempt was made when teaching all aspects of language not to reward imitated utterances, but. rather the child's attempt to produce a spontaneous utterance, thereby reducing the role of imitation in the learning process. however, it is extremely difficult to avoid imitation, and invariably it becomes an inherent part of the teaching programme. bricker & bricker3 make it clear that imitation is one of the more powerful language training tools and the parameters need to be studied if this process is to be used efficiently in training. from the discussion on the auxiliary verb, it can be seen that the ability to imitate a given construction, does not imply that the child is able to use it. ruder and smith 1 8 demonstrated that what is important is that both comprehension and imitation training are required to achieve production, and it is not significant which is done in the initial stages of therapy. the language impaired children studied, and in particular the three subjects whose improvement was minimal, had difficulty in imitating. the normal developing child imitates parents and siblings on verbal and non verbal tasks with ease and pleasure. the ss experienced difficulty on all levels of imitation, and had to be 'taught' to do what is usually spontaneous behaviour. it may prove fruitful to view this inability of the language impaired child from a piagetian point of view. piaget16 regards all symbolic play and mental imagery as being derived from motor imitation, so that the language impaired child's inability to imitate may be representative of a cognitive deficit. if so, language intervention should begin with motor imitation tasks and only later proceed to verbal material. viewing child language syntactically, is limited in that it does not account for the possible restricted nature of the utterances. therefore while five ss were considered to be 'syntactically rehabilitated' (three children still displayed syntactic errors), it must be emphasised that attention was not paid to what the ss did not express. consequently, the language may not have been semantically diversified. as ingram6 states tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en geoorheelkunde. vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 linda narun . . . we may have to shift from worrying about how correctly these children speak to what they have to say. in doing such, we need to face the problems of whether or not teaching syntax will help cognitive development. . . the diagnosis and treatment of w/2-questions illustrates this point. syntactic form of questions improved as a result of the acquisition of the auxiliary. note was not made of the change (if any) that occurred in the type of questions asked. developmentally, some w/2-questions emerge before others. it is the task of the therapist to diagnose and treat any deficits which may occur on this, the semantic level. therefore, in order for any therapy programme to be successful, language must be considered at all levels i.e. phonological, syntactic and semantic. although five ss are described as rehabilitated, this must be regarded tentatively as an assessment of rehabilitation can only be made longitudinally. marge11 outlines five secondary goals of a language programme. one of these is " . . . provision of a linguistic foundation for the further development of language skills, such as reading and writing." the ε feels that in many ways an assessment of rehabilitation is false, unless the therapist is sure that the child will cope in the educational and social fields. the language pathologist must not only ensure that the child is talking, but that the development of secondary symbolic systems, which are dependent on language, is adequate. if our therapy programmes do not facilitate future learning, we must accept lenneberg's assertion that language intervention should be left as late as possible so that maturation can occur (cited by marge11). any programme developed to aid a language impaired child should attempt to isolate these possible future difficulties and provide treatment for them as early as possible. clearly, this area requires a great deal more research and it is possible that the child may require many programmes rather than one focused only on his linguistic abilities. references 1. bloom, l. (1970): language development: form & function in emerging grammars; cambridge, massachusetts & london, england, the m.l.t. press. 2. bloom, l. (1973): one word at a time; the hague, paris: mouton. 3. bricker, w.a. & bricker, d.d. (1974): an early language training strategy. in language perspectives acquisition, retardation and intervention; eds., schiefelbusch, l. & lloyd, l.l. baltimore: university park press. 4. brown, r. (1973): a first language: the early stages. cambridge, massachusetts: harvard university press. 5. ingram, d. (1972a): the acquisition of the english verbal auxiliary & copula in normal and linguistically deviant children. papers & reports in child language. stanford |university. 6. ingram, d. (1972b): the acquisition1 of questions & its relation to cognitive development in normal & linguistically deviant children — a pilotstudy: papers and reports on child language; stanford university. journal of he south african speech and hearing association. vol. 23 december 976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for language impaired children 11 7. jacobs, r.a. & rosenbaum, p.s. (1970): readings in english transformational grammar. massachusetts, toronto, london. ginn & co. 8. jakobson, r. & halle, m. (1971): fundamentals of language. the hague, paris: mouton. 9. klima, ε .s. & bellugi-klima, u. (19 66): syntactic r egularities in the speech of children. in child language: a book of readings. eds. baradon, a. & leopold, w.f. engelwood cliffs, new jersey, prentice hall, 1971: 412-423. reprinted from psycholinguistic papers; eds. lyons, j. & wales, r.j. 10. lee, l.l. (1974): developmental sentence analysis. evanston: northwestern university press. 11. marge, m. (1972): general problem of management and corrective education. in principles of childhood language disabilities; eds. irwin. j.v. & marge, m. new york; appleton-century-'croft, meredith corp. 12. mcneill, d. (1970): the acquisition of language. the study of developmental psycholinguistics: new york, harper & row. 13. menyuk, p. (1969): sentences children use. cambridge, massachusetts: m.l.t. press. 14. morehead, d.m. & ingram, d. (1973): the development of base syntax in normal and linguistically deviant children./. speech hear. research, 16: 330-352. 15. morehead, d.m. & johnson, m. (1972): piaget's theory of intelligence applied to the assessment and treatment of linguistically deviant children. papers & reports on child language. stanford university. 16. piaget, j. (1972): language & thought from the genetic point of view. in language in thinking. ed. adams, p. penguin books ltd. 17. rees, n.s. (1972): bases of decision in language training./. speech hear. dis., 37, 283-304. 18. ruder, k.f. & smith, m.d. (1974): issues in language training. in language perspectives acquisition, retardation and intervention. eds. schiefelbusch, r.l. & lloyd, l.l., baltimore, london & tokyo: university park press. 19. slobin, d.l. (1970): universals of grammatical development in children. in advances in psycholinguistics eds. flores d'arcais, g.b. & levelt, w.j.m., amsterdam, london: north holland publishing co. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en geoorheelkunde vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) philips hearing aid services a division of s.a. philips (pty) ltd. if portable 1 audiometers1 group teaching i i p h i l i p s hearing aid services head office 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street, p.o. box 3069, johannesburg. / philips i ( j w t ) « 9 2 3 ( journal of the south african speech and hearing association. vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) auditory function in a group of adults infected with hiv/aids in gauteng, south africa 17 auditory function in a group of adults infected with hiv/aids in gauteng, south africa katijah khoza & eleanor ross department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract numerous international studies have demonstrated a relationship between hiv/aids and auditory function. the aim of this study was to explore this relationship in a group of adults infected with hiv/aids attending an outpatient clinic in a hospital located in gauteng, south africa. the prevalence of hearing loss; the type, degree and configuration of the hearing loss; the relationship between the hearing symptoms and the progressive stages of the disease, and the type of onset of hearing problems were examined. the results of the study indicated a prevalence rate of hearing loss which was as high as 23% in the sample surveyed. the types of hearing loss included conductive and sensorineural, while the degree of severity ranged from slight to profound in nature. the configuration of the hearing loss was not frequency-range-specific, and the degree of severity did not seem to worsen with the progression of the hiv/aids disease. however, there did seem to be an increase in the occurrence of sensorineural hearing loss with the deterioration of patients' immunological status. analysis of patients' audiological results along with their case history data suggested that their hearing loss may have been caused by opportunistic infections and/or their treatments. these results are discussed in terms of their implications for the clinical management of patients with hiv/aids; education of team members; and policy formulation.· key words: immune cell count; aids; ototoxic; serology; hearing loss introduction the acquired immune deficiency syndrome (aids) and the human immunodeficiency virus (hiv) that causes it seem to j have arguably created more challenges to science and! medicine than any other single disease. moreover, the aids epidemic is now in its third decade and would seem to have become one of the most important public health problems in south africa (south african department of health, 2000). hiv is reported' to cause a total breakdown of the body's natural immune system by reducing the immune cell count (bankaitis, 1996). such a reduction leads to the development of various diseases,, which does not occur similarly in all patients infected with the virus. bankaitis (1996) reports that many of the patients with the virus remain, asymptomatic and maintain normal immune cell count for long periods of time, and they are therefore described as falling outside the documented clinical definition of aids. for patients to be diagnosed with aids, they must either have an extremely low thelper lymphocyte count or a cd4+ blood; count (below 200 /mm3 of blood) or present with at least one aidsdefining condition. the american center for disease control and prevention (cdc) classifies hiv infected patients into three distinct groups according to their cd4+ blood count, where patients with cd4+ count greater than 500/mm3 are classified as asymptomatic (stage 1), patients with1 cd4+ count between 200 and 499/mm3 as symptomatic (stage 2), and patients with cd4+ count less than 200/mm3 as having full blown aids (stage 3) (bankaitis, 1996; chandrasekhar, connelly, brahmbhatt, shah, kloser, & baredes, 2000). from the first diagnosis of hiv/aids and throughout most of the epidemic the concern of most people has been to sustain life by preserving the immune system and fighting off primary, life-threatening infections. however, as a result of recent advances in treatment, the focus of therapy has shifted to a more rehabilitative approach with the emphasis on enhancing quality of life (friedman & noffsinger, 1998). in addition to treating life-threatening infections there is an increasing awareness of the need to consider the impact of hiv/aids on cognitive, motor, and sensory domains, with hearing changes being a potential presentation at any stage*of the disease (larson, 1998). • as early as 1987 sooy reported on abnormal audiologic findings of thresholds worse than 25db hl, with a high frequency sloping configuration on pure tone audiometry in patients with aids. more recently, a study of black female hiv positive patients in a south african hospital reported that the most prevalent degree and type of hearing loss found was slight to mild and sensorineural in nature (fuzani, 1999). chandrasekhar et al. (2000) reported that the high frequencies of 4000 hz and 8000 hz were significantly elevated relative to other frequencies. such results are in line with those reported by sooy (1987) that indicate worsening hearing loss in high frequencies. with regard to otologic symptoms, contradictory research findings, mostly from american studies, have been reported. for example, rosenberg, schneider, and die sid-afrikaansetydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) katijah khoza and eleanor ross 18 cohen (1985) reported a total absence of otologic symptoms in american patients who presented with head and neck manifestations of the disease, while booth (1997) noted an increasing trend in the number of people living with hiv/aids who presented with otologic symptoms. lalwani and sooy (1992) maintained that ear related manifestations of hiv/aids occur to a lesser degree relative to other head and neck complaints. these discrepancies in findings could be attributed to different methodological steps employed in these studies. the management of hiv/aids, which involves medications such as azidothymidine (azt) and radiation treatment, has been reported to have an adverse effect on the auditory function of individuals infected with hiv/aids (bankaitis & larson, 1998; schountz, 1998). however, more recently, chandrasekhar et al. (2000) reported that the sensorineural hearing loss detected in their outpatient subjects did not correlate with their routine hiv/aids medications used. despite these contradictory findings, changes in audiological function attributable to hiv/aids, and the medications used to treat the condition and its. associated opportunistic infections, are likely to increase the demand for audiologists to work with patients with hiv/aids. whilst it is evident that a cure for aids, does not appear to be imminent, treatment strategies have been improving rapidly suggesting that 'long-term therapeutic protocols for the management of infections are likely to be developed in the near future (tshabalala-msimang, 1999). regensberg and maartens (1999) stated that the drugs currently available have significantly improved the prognosis and quality of life for patients living with hiv/aids thereby allowing them to continue to be productive members of society. improved hearing and enhanced communication have the potential of contributing positively to the patients' quality of life, and hence ensuring their ability to remain productive members of society. while hearing loss related to hiv/aids is receiving increasing attention in the international literature, the reported prevalence rates may not necessarily apply to south africa. in view of the increase in the number of south africans who are reported to be infected (south african department of heath, 2000), as well as the researcher's clinical experience, and review of audiology records of patients with hiv/aids at the research site, it was hypothesized that hearing loss might be the presenting problem in some cases of undiagnosed hiv infection. real, thomas, and gerwins (1987) support the hypothesis that sensorineural hearing loss may in some cases be the presenting complaint of undiagnosed hiv infection. furthermore, because the head and neck region has been reported as a common anatomical site for medical conditions associated with hiv/aids (rosenberg et al., 1985), audiologists, as part of the head and neck team, need to contribute to the theoretical knowledge about audiological manifestations of hiv/aids. such knowledge may increase the likelihood of appropriate assessments leading to accurate diagnoses and management (noffsinger & friedman, 1996). for these reasons, this study was conducted. methodology aim of the study the aim of the study was to explore the auditory function in a group of adults infected with hiv/aids attending a hospital outpatient clinic in gauteng, south africa. the six specific sub-aims were to determine the prevalence of hearing loss in the sample; to assess the type, degree, configuration and symmetry of the hearing loss; to determine the relationship between the hearing symptoms and the progressive stages of hiv/aids; to explore the nature of onset of hearing loss (e.g. sudden vs. gradual/progressive onset); to determine the relationship between the nature of onset of sensorineural hearing loss and the severity of the hearing loss; and lastly, to relate the hiv/aids signs and symptoms of each subject to the nature of the hearing loss. research design the study was exploratory in nature. the design utilised was non-experimental and observational in nature with no control group and non-randomization of subjects (mccall, 1990). the aim was to determine whether there was a relationship between hiv/aids and auditory function. the subjects' age, clinical stage of the hiv/aids, and co-occurring diseases (e.g. tb, syphilis, middle ear pathology) were the independent variables, while the audiological measures (otoscopy, impedance audiometry, pure tone audiometry, abr) were the dependent variables. description of subjects subject selection criteria the south african population that is predominantly infected with hiv/aids is unique in that it includes mineworkers (who are exposed to noise in the workplace), and hiv/aids co-occurs with diseases such as tb and syphilis (south african department of health, 2000). the treatment of hiv/aids, the opportunistic infections and co-occurring symptoms may impact on hearing (bankaitis & schountz, 1998; larson, 1998). it was therefore difficult to specify selection criteria without excluding" almost all patients attending the hiv/aids clinic. as this was an exploratory study which endeavoured to represent the south african hiv/aids population, it was considered undesirable to isolate the impact of hiv/aids on hearing. such a sampling1 procedure has disadvantages relating to the presence of confounding variables e.g. ototoxic drugs for tb, noise exposure, syphilis, etc. however, inclusion and documentation of such variables in the hiv/aids population might yield important information. consequently, the subject selection criteria were: > subjects' hiv/aids status needed to have been confirmed by serology studies. > subjects needed to be between the ages of 18 and 55 years. there are reported age differences in presentation of the virus (matkin, diefendorf, & erenberg, 1998). > subjects were not to have presbycusis. presbycusis could confound the results since it also presents as a sloping high frequency sensori-neural hearing loss ' (snhl) (katz, 1994). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) auditory function in a group of adults infected with hiv/aids in gauteng, south africa 19 > the subjects had to be alert, oriented, able to provide informed consent, and to participate in an audiological assessment. sampling procedure a convenience sampling technique was utilised in recruiting subjects for the study. subjects volunteered to have their hearing status evaluated in response to notices that were posted at the hiv/aids clinic inviting them to participate in the study, and in response to the researcher's verbal explanations of the purpose of the project at this clinic. subject description a total of 150 subjects, including both males and females, comprised the research sample. a profile of the subjects is set out in table 1. the subjects spanned all three cdc stages and were also categorised into four age groups as depicted in table 1. inspection of the subjects' data revealed demographic similarities between them and the general south african population infected with hiv/aids (crewe, 1999; tshabalala-msimang, 1999). firstly, crewe (1999), supported by tshabalala-msimang (1999) maintains that women have the highest hiv prevalence rates in .the country. the gender bias in the prevalence rates of the disease was also evident in this sample, with a much higher number of participants in the study being female. secondly, the distribution of subjects across the different age ranges mirrors those of the country. most subjects were between 20 and 45 years of age, which is the most vulnerable age group for hiv/aids (tshabalala-msimang, 1999). table 1. demographic and medical profile of all subjects in the study (n = 150) factor sub-category number age ranges (years) 18-25 26-35 36-45 46-55 19 77 45 9 gender male female 54 96 ethnic group black white coloured indian 147 2 1 0 cd4+ count (/mm3) (cdc stages) asymptomatic (stage 1) > 500/mmj symptomatic (stage 2) 200 499/mm3 aids (stage ,3) < 200/mm3 38 (25%) 52 (35%) 60 (40%) hearing status κ 1 1 1 normal: stage 1 stage 2 stage 3 abnormal: stage 1 stage 2 stage 3 35 39, 41 total: 115 (77%) 3 " 13 19 total: 35 (23%) tinnitus j present •absent 34 (23%) 116 (77%) vertigo ! i present absent 14 (9%) 136 (91%) symmetry of hearing loss (n=35) i unilateral bilateral 10 (29%) ' · . 25 (71%). type of onset of hearing loss sudden gradual 16 19 type of hearing loss conductive sensorineural mixed 4 21 10 possible aetiology of hearing loss • meningitis oto/syphilis otitis media ' encephalitis tb treatment 9 13 14 1 2 hearing status and possible aetiological factors i •subjects with possible causal factors (i.e. tb, syphilis) but normal hearing (n = 115) subjects with possible causal factors and abnormal hearing1 (n = 35) 14 (12%) (14 of the 115 subjects with normal hearing) 32 (91,42%) (32 of. the 35 subjects .with hearing loss) noise exposure ' present absent 23 (15%) . 127 (85%) die suid-afrikaanse.tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings. vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) katijah khoza and eleanor ross 20 testing procedures the testing procedure was conducted in four phases: phase i prior to commencement of the study, permission to conduct the research project was obtained from the university of the witwatersrand medical ethics committee for research on human subjects, the hospital superintendent and relevant heads of departments. thereafter, the researcher discussed details regarding the project with the medical team at the hiv/aids clinic, and enlisted their co-operation in informing their patients about the study. informed consent was obtained from all individuals volunteering to participate in the study. a case history form (available on request from researchers) was used to record the case history, audiological data, and medical variables that could have an impact on the results of the study. case history form the devised case history form consisted of the following sections: > demographic information: information obtained included subjects' ethnic group, age and gender and was used to determine the representative nature of the sample. > history of noise exposure: information on noise exposure was obtained to differentiate between hearing loss that was noise-induced versus hiv/aids related. the data gathered included the length of time the subject was exposed to noise, the type of noise, and the intensity of the noise as per subjects' report. chronic and consistent exposure to noise causes hearing loss particularly in the high frequencies, generally seen as a dip at 4000 hz (katz, 1994). > history of tinnitus and vertigo: tinnitus and vertigo are associated with hearing loss (katz, 1994), and the subjects were asked if they experienced these symptoms which were described by the researcher. > hearing status: information on family history of hearing impairment, previous audiologic assessments, time since onset and nature (sudden, gradual) of onset, progression of the hearing loss, current hearing status, and laterality of the hearing loss was obtained from subjects. such questions form part of the audiologic evaluation (bess & humes, 1990; schuknecht, 1993). > medical history: the subjects' medical records were reviewed to obtain information on the following: • the clinical or serological evidence of syphilis as this disease is known to cause hearing loss (darmstadt & harris, 1989). • case history factors that may have contributed to a hearing loss e.g. history of prior ear disease; head trauma; use of ototoxic drugs; diseases such as tb, cancer, etc. (katz, 1994; lalwani & sooy, 1992). • treatment history that could contribute to hearing loss. two major forms of such treatment are radiation therapy and azt (bankaitis & schountz, 1998; larson, 1998). the • the different cdc categories (asymptomatic stage; symptomatic stage; and full-blown aids) (bankaitis, 1996). case history information was obtained by interviewing each subject prior to the audiologic evaluation. individuals who were literate in english completed parts of the case history form (except for the medical information) on their own. the medical history information (e.g. medical diagnosis, serology information cd4+ count, ent diagnosis, etc) was obtained from the medical records for all subjects. phase ii following infection control measures proposed by kemp and roeser (1998), basic audiological information was obtained via otoscopy, tympanometry, and pure tone audiometry for each subject. hearing thresholds were obtained by both air conduction and bone conduction so as to differentiate between conductive and sensorineural hearing loss as per katz's protocol (1994). the objective of the otoscopic evaluation, using a welch allyn otoscope, was to examine the subjects' ears for the presence of impacted wax, otitis externa, possible otitis media, perforated tympanic membranes, collapsed ear canals, presence of any growths and so forth (friedman & arnold, 1993). findings were then confirmed by the otolaryngologists at the hospital. impedance audiometry in the form of tympanometry assessed the status and integrity of middle ear functioning. tympanometry has been reported to be superior to routine otoscopy in correctly predicting the absence or presence of middle ear effusions (silman & silverman, 1991). the use of both assessment procedures in a complementary fashion was deemed necessary for this study as the results were combined to provide valuable redundant information to facilitate accurate diagnoses (wiley & fowler, 1997). phase iii auditory brainstem response (abr) measurements were obtained for subjects with sensorineural hearing loss (snhl) in order to determine possible retrocochlear involvement (hall, 1992; hood, 1998). it was acknowledged that the abr should be used in a test battery approach rather than in isolation to determine site of auditory pathology. the test battery includes otoacoustic emissions, speech discrimination measures, loudness balance tests, metz test-acoustic reflex, short increment sensitivity index, tone decay, and acoustic reflex decay (katz, 1994). however, these site of lesion tests were not performed because of the lack of appropriate testing tools at the time of the study. therefore the abr, which has been documented as an objective and sensitive tool for assessing the neural intergrity of the auditory pathway was utilised (hall, 1992; hood, 1998). hall's (1992) protocol was-adopted for. abr testing for neurodiagnosis of eighth nerve or auditory brainstem· dysfunction. responses · were collected three times for each ear so as to ensure reliability of the measurement procedures and that the artefacts were of acceptable levels. the criteria for determining abnormal abr were those advocated by hall (1992), hood (1998), silman ind silverman (1991), and spehlmann (1985). south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) auditory function in a group of adults infected with hiv/aids in gauteng, south africa 21 speech testing was not one of the tests selected to form part of the test battery due to the paucity of s t a n d a r d i s e d word lists for the multilingual south african population. in addition, it was envisaged that there could be poor performance on speech tests because of differences in dialect, word-meaning, and word familiarity levels. such problems have been reported by wilson, jones, and fridjhon (1998) who maintained that subjects' educational level may influence their linguistic performance. phase iv this phase involved re-inspection of the data for the subgroup with abnormal hearing. all persons whose hearing loss was not directly attributable to hiv/aids, but rather to confounding variables such as ototoxicity and diseases such as meningitis were excluded. the purpose of this exercise was to try to establish the number of subjects where there might be a direct link between hiv/aids and impaired hearing function. data analysis procedures and statistical following consultations with statisticians it was concluded that the nature of the data collected precluded inferential statistical analysis, and therefore descriptive statistical analyses were performed. each subject was classified as either having normal or abnormal hearing following the audiological evaluation. normal hearing was regarded as responses at and better than 25 db hl, with abnormal results being thresholds worse than 25db hl (katz, 1994; silman & silverman, 1991). the degree of hearing loss was determined using silman and silverman's (1991) classification of "magnitude of hearing impairment". this classification system, supported by katz (1994), advocates that impaired hearing function begins at an average hearing level of 25 db hl. abnormal hearing was further categorized into type of hearing loss; • normal hearing s a b n o r m a l hearing figure 1: the prevalence of hearing loss in hiv/aids subjects (n = 150) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 49, 2002 severity; and the nature of onset of the hearing loss. all case history factors were recorded and analysed along with the audiological results in order to obtain a comprehensive evaluation. the aibnormal hearing results were then group matched to the different cdc stages to determine relationships between hearing loss and stages of hiv/aids. furthermore, the data were examined to establish if there was any relationship between the nature of onset (sudden or gradual) of snhl and the severity of the hearing loss. results and discussion according to the research literature, auditory abnormalities have been reported in persons with varying degrees of hiv infection, in both symptomatic and asymptomatic patients (bankaitis & keith, 1995; bankaitis, 1996; chandrasekhar et al., 2000; fuzani, 1999). similar findings were obtained in the present study and have particular relevance to the field of audiology. the results are discussed in accordance with the sub-aims of the study. the prevalence of hearing loss of the 150 subjects tested, 115 (77%) had normal hearing, and 35 (23%) presented with a hearing loss as depicted in figure 1. thus 23% of subjects had both otologic and audiological symptoms that included tinnitus, vertigo, and hearing losses with varying nature and severity. these results appear to be consistent with some internationally published studies on otologic and audiologic manifestations of hiv/aids (bankaitis, 1996; bankaitis & keith, 1995; birchall et al, 1992; chandrasekhar et al, 2000). the general trend reported in the american literature suggests that the prevalence of otologic manifestations of hiv/aids is relatively small (sooy, 1987), with the exception of flower (1991) who reported auditory abnormality prevalence rates in adults with aids to be as high as 75%. this difference in results could be related to variations in subject selection criteria, sample sizes, and the otologic versus audiologic nature of the studies. more recently, booth (1997) reported an increase, in the united states, in the number of patients with otologic symptoms associated with aids. birchall, wight, french, and smith (1992) reported hearing loss (not type-specific) on pure tone testing in 39% of patients who were hivpositive and syphilis-negative with no neurological symptoms, while sooy (1987) described hearing impairment greater than 25db hl on pure tone testing in 49% of the subjects. a surprising finding was that in the presence of these abnormal audiologic findings in sooy's study, the speech discrimination scores were reported to have consistently been above 82% for all patients evaluated, with the majority of the patients obtaining scores that were better than 90%. consistent with the aforementioned studies, the current study also reported the presence of otologic disease in some subjects who presented with head and neck symptoms. however, this finding differs from those studies (marcusen & sooy, 1985; rosenberg et al, 1985) reporting a total absence of otologic signs in the adult population. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) these studies concluded that while there was an extremely low incidence of otologic disease in adults, there was a much higher incidence in the paediatric population. the high incidence of conductive hearing loss in paediatric patients was attributed, in part, to the high incidence of serous otitis media which occurred in up to 80% of cases (smith & canalis, 1989). rosenberg, schneider, and cohen (1985) reviewed medical records of 102 adult patients with aids and found that although 71% of the patients had symptoms localised in the head and neck, none had otologic signs and symptoms. similar results were reported by marcusen and sooy (1985) who also could not find any otologic findings in 165 aids infected patients they evaluated. lalwani and sooy (1992) maintain that otologic manifestations associated with hiv/aids do occur but are less common than other head and neck complaints. methodological differences such as retrospective case reviews (rosenberg et al., 1985) versus prospective evaluations of subjects (marcusen & sooy, 1985) could have contributed to the contradictory findings. the high incidence of hearing loss in the studied sample provides strong support for the view that audiologists should be involved in the assessment and management of patients with hiv/aids (larson, 1998). the type, degree, configuration, and symmetry of the hearing loss type of hearing loss katijah khoza and eleanor ross 22 as can be seen in figure 2, snhl was the most commonly occurring type, while conductive hearing loss (chl) was the least frequent. of those with a snhl 11 (52%) were possibly cochlear, and 10 (48%) were possibly retrocochlear; and 10 (29%) had mixed hearing loss (mhl) as indicated in figure 2. these results concur with the literature in that any type of hearing loss i.e. conductive, sensory/neural, or central hearing loss may be seen in hiv/aids (chandrasekhar et al., 2000; friedmann & noffsinger, 1998). however, the current results differ from the documented prevalence rates of types of hearing loss (friedmann & noffsinger, 1998; gold & tami, ί 998; lalwani & sooy, 1992). the most common otologic problems reported were serous otitis media and recurrent acute otitis media, and were predominantly related to eustachian tube dysfunction implying that conductive hearing loss was the most common. in this study, chl was the least common type of problem seen. there were slightly lower rates of occurrence of snhl (between 20 and 50%) reported by gold and tami (1998). the higher incidence in the current study could be related to sampling differences in that subjects with syphilis were excluded from some of the international studies. degree of hearing loss subjects had varying degrees of hearing loss as illustrated in figure 3. mild hearing loss (34%) was most common, followed closely by the profound hearing impairment (27%). table 2 indicates that of the subjects 70 8 6 0 1 i 50 j 3 ι ο 8 40 & 30 u) ro φ 2 0 ο φ ο1 0 -j 0 11 60 49 29 2 0 c h l s h l m h l t y p e s of h e a r i n g l o s s figure 2: types of hearing loss in the group of subjects with hearing loss (n = 35) key: rpta = right ear pure tone audiometry lpta = left ear pure tone audiometry mhl = mixed hearing loss chl = conductive hearing loss shl = sensorineural hearing loss the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) function in a group of adults infected with hiv/aids in gauteng, south africa 23 • φ ο c φ 3 ο ο ο φ σ> re c φ ο φ ο . 40 35 30 25 20 15 10 5 0 s / m μ m / s e s e m 23 20 7 7 17 27 ι • 34 10 7 : io 17 21 ! d e g r e e of h e a r i n g l o s s figure 3: degree of hearing loss in the subjects with hearing loss (n = 35) key: rpta = right ear pure tone audiometry s = mild hearing loss μ = moderate hearing loss se = severe hearing loss with snhl, most of them (67%) presented with a severe to profound hearing loss. these results differ from those reported in the literature where severity ranged from mild (fuzani, 1999) to severe (sooy, 1987), with little mention of profound hearing loss. sooy (1987) reported that subjects presented with hearing losses greater than 25db, typically ranging between 30 and 50db hl at 8000 hz. the degree of hearing loss reported in this study suggests that audiologic management of patients with hiv/aids should commence promptly so that early intervention can.be instituted, thereby preventing further deterioration of hearing thresholds over time. lpta = left ear pure tone audiometry s/m =mild to moderate hearing loss m/se = moderate to severe hearing loss ρ = profound hearing loss 1997). these results suggest that all frequencies may be affected equally or to varying degrees. previous studies, however, e.g. chandrasekhar et al. (2000), gold and tami (1998), and lalwani and sooy (1992) reported a sloping high frequency hearing loss, mostly snhl. aetiology of the hearing loss could be a contributory factor to the differences noted. symmetry of hearing loss as can be seen from table'3, of the 35 subjects with hearing losses, 29% had unilateral hearing impairment while 71% presented with bilateral hearing loss, suggesting that hearing loss can be unilateral or bilateral in adults with hiv/aids. the high prevalence of bilateral hearing loss among subjects suggests that . hearing impairment probably occurs more frequently and with greater severity than anticipated in the patients with hiv/aids. bilateral hearing loss has been reported to table 2. type of onset of hearing loss in subjects with hearing loss (n = 35) configuration of hearing loss no typical pattern of configuration of hearing loss could be established. of the subjects with a hearing loss, only 14% presented with a sloping/high frequency hearing loss. the remaining 86% presented with flat and/or irregular audiograms, which related well to the possible causes of hearing loss (e.g., meningitis and otitis media which do not only affect high frequencies but low and mid frequencies as well) (booth, sudden onset gradual/progressive onset total = 16 total = 19 > 13 (81 %) severe to profound snhl > 3 mild-moderate hearing loss > 2 (10%) severe snhl > 7 (37%) mild-moderate snhl > 10 (53%) conductive and mixed die sid-afrikaansetydskrifvir kommunikasieawykings vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) katijah khoza and eleanor ross 24 impact more severely on communication than unilateral hearing loss, and therefore requires prompt diagnosis and management (katz, 1994; silman & silverman, 1991). early intervention in terms of medical management and/or amplification would enhance patients' communication abilities, and faciltate compliance with any verbally prescribed medical treatment. hence, enhanced communication could potentially improve not only the social, academic, and vocational quality of the patients' life, but also their general health. the relationship between hearing loss and the progressive stages of hiv i aids as illustrated in figure 4, there was a trend towards an increase in the number of occurrences of snhl from stage 1 (asymptomatic) to stage 3 (fullblown aids), but there was no definitive relationship to the 3 cdc categories for chl and mhl. these findings are supported by evidence which suggests a relationship between audiological manifestations and the progression of the hiv/aids disease (birchall et al., 1992; chandrasekhar et al., 2000; lalwani & sooy 1992). the increase in the occurrence of snhl with advanced stages of the disease in the current study may be attributed to the progressive decline in the immunologic status which increases susceptibility to the neurotropic nature of the disease and to opportunistic infections, which have been found to cause hearing loss (friedmann & arnold, 1993; real et al., 1987; schuknecht, 1993). type of onset of hearing symptoms as indicated in table 2, similar numbers of subjects presented with sudden and with gradual onset of hearing loss. several studies have reported sudden onset of snhl in patients with hiv/aids (real et al., 1987; timon & walsh, 1989). smith and canalis (1989) reported that snhl can occasionally be of a sudden onset but is commonly rapidly progressive. chandrasekhar et al. (2000) also reported similar findings with 3% presenting with sudden onset, 21% with gradual onset, while the largest number demonstrated intermittent onset. type of onset of snhl and its relationship to degree of the hearing loss as further indicated in table 2, sudden onset of hearing loss occurred most frequently in subjects who presented with severe to profound snhl, while gradual onset was mostly found in subjects who presented with conductive and/or mixed hearing losses. real et al. (1987) suggested that sudden snhl (temporary or permanent) was frequently caused by viral agents, in as many as 33% of cases. the relationship between hiv/aids signs and symptoms and the nature of the hearing loss detailed descriptive analysis of the audiologic evaluation results together with the documented case history information where medical diagnoses had been confirmed by ear, nose and throat specialists revealed the following: > patients who presented with snhl had documented medical histories of meningitis (aseptic; cryptococcal; and viral); infections (syphilis and otosyphilis; encephalitis); and histories of ototoxic medication used in the treatment of tb and other opportunistic infections. three patients had possible noiseinduced hearing loss due to industrial noise exposure. > patients who presented with chl and mhl figure 4: hearing loss and its relationship to the cd4+ count in the subjects with hearing loss (n = 35) key: chl = conductive hearing loss ' mhl = mixed hearing loss : shl = sensorineural hearing loss 1 = stage 1 of hiv/aids (asymptomatic) 2 = stage 2 of hiv/aids (symptomatic) 3 = stage 3 of hiv/aids (aids) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) auditory function in a group of adults infected with hiv/aids in gauteng, south africa 25 had a history of chronic suppurative otitis media, and otitis media with effusion. the above results are consistent with the welldocumented causes of hearing loss in the population with hiv/aids (friedmann & arnold, 1993; smith & canalis, 1989). it has been reported that decreased cellmediated immunity, recurrent viral· infections, nonmalignant lymphoid hyperplasia of the adenoids, nasopharyngeal tumours," sinusitis, or allergic autoimmune reaction to hiv can all lead to poor eustachian· tube function and middle ear effusions (friedmann & arnold, 1993; gold & tami, 1998; lalwani & sooy, 1992). chandrasekhar et al. (2000) reported that 23% of adults with hiv presented with otitis media which is highly uncommon in adults who are hiv-negative, and thus suggest that conductive hearing loss can be expected as a presenting problem .in hivinfected patients. the causes of snhl (which involve the central nervous system (cns) and the sensory end organs) in hiv/aids include cranial neuropathy due to meningitis (aseptic, cryptococcal, and viral) or lymphoma; viruses (cytomegalovirus; hepatitis b; herpes simplex and syphilis -otosyphilis and neurosyphilis; herpes zoster; and toxoplasma) which are conditions reported to be more common in patients with hiv than in healthy individuals; ototoxic drugs used in the treatment of opportunistic infections and neoplasms; and intracranial events, such as encephalitis and haemorrhage (bankaitis & schountz, 1998; booth, 1997; friedmann & arnold, 1993; larson, 1998; real et al., 1987; schuknecht, 1993; smith & canalis, 1989). in this study, meningitis and syphilis were the two major possible causes of snhl with encephalitis and ototoxicity being minor causes. the three types of meningitis (aseptic, cryptococcal, and viral) reported, all have an effect on the auditory system (schuknecht, 1993). cryptococcal meningitis is an opportunistic infection that has its primary focus in the lung with spread mainly to the meninges (schuknecht, 1993). this infection causes similar tissue damage both iri people with and without hiv infection, with the exception that table 3: summary of case history data for subjects with hearing loss (n=35) factor sub-category number percentage cd4+ count (/mm3) asymptomatic (stage 1) >' 500/mm3 3 9 symptomatic (stage 2) 200 499/ mm3 13 37 aids (stage 3) < 200/mm3 19 54 . type of hearing loss conductive hearing loss 4 11 sensorineural hearing loss 21 60 mixed hearing loss 10 29 type of onset of sudden 16 46 hearing loss gradual 1954 symmetry /of hearing unilateral 10 29 loss j bilateral 25 71 possible aetiology of . meningitis 9 26 hearing loss * oto/syphilis 13 37 ί otitis media 14 40 s encephalitis 1 3 , • tb treatment 2 9 possible site of lesion cochlear 11 52 of snhl (n=21) ι retrocochlear 10 1 48 degree of snhl mildmoderate 7 . 33 (n=21) severe-profound 14 67 . tinnitus present 16 46 absent 19 54 vertigo present 9 26 absent 26 74 noise exposure present 6 17 1 absent 29 83 note: * % scores do not add up to 100% as some subjects presented with more than one possible aetiological factor die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) in people with hiv, the damage tends to be more extensive and destructive (larson, 1998). booth (1997) reports cryptococcal meningitis as being increasingly associated with sudden onset snhl with the incidence as high as 27%. the hearing loss is reported to be mostly retrocochlear in nature. in the current study, of the 3 subjects with cryptococcal meningitis, one subject had a possible cochlear site of lesion, while the other two subjects had possible retrocochlear lesions. real et al. (1987) reported that the organism in cryptococcal meningitis has been found in both the cochlear and vestibular nerves, and in the end organs, with severe damage to the organ of corti. they further report that neuronal loss with good hair cell preservation can also be found. thus they suggested that hearing loss due to cryptococcal meningitis could be either cochlear or retrocochlear in nature. syphilis and otosyphilis have been linked to sudden or gradual onset of snhl (schuknecht, 1993). research has suggested that otosyphilis affects the cochlear (booth, 1997; friedmann & arnold, 1993; smith & canalis, 1989). in contrast, the current study had four patients with possible retrocochlear lesions. it could be speculated that such a finding was possibly related to difficulty in diagnosing otosyphilis (friedmann & arnold, 1993)", which might lead to an incorrect diagnosis, especially if a differential diagnosis has not been done to exclude other systemic processes that occur in the hiv/aids population. retrocochlear lesions may also be possible since hiv changes the course of syphilis by hastening the development of neurosyphilis with resultant neural hearing loss (smith & canalis, 1989). ototoxicity can occur in this population since the patients' drug regimen often involves potentially ototoxic medications. however, this issue requires more research as bankaitis and schountz (1998) reported that experimental antiretroviral drugs (with undocumented or unknown side effects) could contribute to hearing loss, while chandrasekhar et al. (2000) reported no correlation between sensorineural hearing loss and routine medications. medical histories of normal hearing subjects vs. subjects with hearing losses: of the total of 150 subjects evaluated, 115 subjects presented with normal hearing thresholds. in phase iv of the study, it became evident that the major differentiating factor between the two groups (normal hearing group and the hearing impaired group) was the presence of opportunistic infections. subjects with normal hearing had no opportunistic infections and other possible causes of hearing loss, except in 12% of the subjects. there was, however, strong evidence suggesting that hearing loss, in this study, was probably due to opportunistic infections and their treatment rather than from hiv/aids alone. of the 23% of subjects with hearing loss, almost all presented with a history of opportunistic infections and other aetiological factors associated, with hearing loss in patients with hiv/aids. hence, the hearing loss in hiv/aids subjects may not be solely attributable to hiv/aids but to a combination of factors. katijah khoza and eleanor ross 26 conclusion numerous internationally published studies have demonstrated a correlation between hearing loss and hiv/aids (bankaitis, 1996; birchall et al., 1992; chandrasekhar et al., 2000; gold & tami, 1998; sooy, 1987). findings from the present exploratory-descriptive study conducted on a local south african sample also revealed a relationship between hiv/aids and auditory function. the estimated prevalence of hearing loss among adults attending an hiv/aids clinic in a government hospital in-gauteng was around 23%. the hearing loss occurred at any stage of the disease with varying degrees of severity and types. in addition, it is suggested that a diagnosis of hiv/aids be suspected if a patient presents with a sudden hearing loss, with no clear aetiology, as it could be the first symptom in some undiagnosed hiv/aids patients. results from the present study have the potential to contribute toward enhancing audiological assessment and management of patients infected with this virus, particularly in the current era of financial constraints across services in provincial hospitals. however, these results need to be considered in relation to issues identified in the project's research design and analysis. by ensuring that appropriate management in the form of correct hearing aid fitting where progression of the hearing loss is known or suspected, regular rechecks of hearing status for possible adjustment of hearing aid settings so as to ensure maximum benefit from amplification, and visual training where hearing aids are of no or little benefit to patients, audiologists will be contributing enormously towards providing efficacious care to patients with hiv/aids. in conclusion, the results from the present study suggest that the audiologic presentation of patients with hiv/aids is consistent with their immunocompromised status. susceptibility to opportunistic infections increases chances of quantifiable audiological sequelae. references bankaitis, a.e. (1996). audiological changes attributable to hiv. audiology today, 8(6), 1416. bankaitis, a.f., & keith, r.w. (1995). audiologic changes associated with hiv infection. ear nose and throat journal, 74, 353-359. bankaitis, a.e., & schountz, t. (1998). hiv-related ototoxicity. seminars in hearing, 19, 155-163. birchall, m., wight, r., french, p., cockbain, z., & smith, s. (1992). auditory function in patients infected with the human immunodeficiency virus. clinical otolaryngology, 17, 117-121. booth, j.b. (1997). scott-brown's otolaryngology: otology. (6th ed), boston: butterworthheinemann. chandrasekhar, s.s., connelly, p.e., brahmbhatt, s.s., chetan, b.s., shah, s., kloser, p.c., & baredes, s. (2000). otologic and audiologic evaluation of human immunodeficiency virusinfected patients. american journal of otolaryngology, 2/(1), 1-9. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) auditory function in a group of adults infected wi crewe, m. (1999, november). aids, activism and the role of the universities. paper presented at the conference of the social, demographic, and development impact of aids: commonwealth universities respond, durban, south africa. darnstadt, g. l., & harris, j.p. (1989). luetic hearing loss: clinical presentation, diagnosis, and treatment. american journal of otolaryngology, 10:410. department of health. (2000). hiv/a1ds/std strategic plan for south africa 2000-2005. rsa. friedmann, i., & arnold, w. (1993). pathology of the ear. london: churchill livingstone. friedman, j.l., & noffsinger, d. (1998). hearing loss associated with hiv/aids: social, cultural, and political issues. seminars in hearing, 19, 205214. fuzani, p. (1999). the type, degree and prevalence of hearing loss in black female hiv positive patients at various clinical stages of infection in south africa. undergraduate unpublished report, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. gold, s., & tami, t.a. (1998). otolaryngology manifestations of hiv/aids. seminars in hearing, 19, 165-175. hall, j.w. (1992). handbook of auditory evoked responses. boston: allyn & bacon. hood, l.j. (1998). clinical applications of the auditory brainstem response. san diego: singular publishing. jacobson, j.t., & northern, j.l. (1990). diagnostic audiology. boston: allyn and bacon. katz, j. (1994). handbook of clinical audiology. baltimore: williams & wilklns. kemp, r.j., & roeser, r.j. (1998). infection control for audiologists. seminars in hearing, 19(2), 195203. lalwani, a.k., & sooy, c.d. (1992). otologic and neurologic manifestations of acquired immunodeficiency syndrome. the otolaryngological clinics of north america, 25(6), 1183-1198. larson, c.r. (1998). \ htv-1 and communication disorders: what speech and hearing professionals need to know. london: singular publishing inc. ' marcusen, d.c., & sooy, c.d. (1985). otolaryngologic and head and neck manifestations of acquired immunodeficiency syndrome (aids). laryngoscope, 95, 401-405. hiv/aids in gauteng, south africa 27 matkin, n.d., diefendorf, a.o., & erenberg, a. (1998). children: hiv/aids and hearing loss. seminars in hearing, 19, 143-153. mccall, r.b. (1990). fundamental statistics for behavioral sciences (4th ed). new york. noffsinger, d., & friedman, j. (1996). audiology and the person with hearing loss associated with hiv/aids. audiology today, 8(6), 12-13. real, r., thomas, m., & gerwins, j.m. (1987). sudden hearing loss and acquired immunodeficiency syndrome. otolaryngology head and neck surgery, 97, 4099-4120. regensberg, l., & maartens, g. (1999). antiretroviral therapy in adults. modern medicine of south africa, 30-38. rosenberg, r.a., schneider, k.l., & cohen, n.l. (1985). head and neck presentations of acquired immunodeficiency syndrome. otolaryngology head neck surgery, 93, 700-705. schuknecht, h.f. (1993). pathology of the ear (2nd ed). london: lea & febiger. silman, s., & silverman, c.a. (1991). auditory diagnosis: principles and applications. san diego: academic press. smith, m.e., & canalis, r.f. (1989). otologic manifestations of aids: the otosyphilis connection. laryngoscope, 99, 365-372. sooy, c.d. (1987). the impact of aids on otolaryngology head and neck surgery. advances in otolaryngological head & neck surgery, 1, 1-28. spehlmann, r. (1985). evoked potential primer: visual, auditory, and somatosensory evoked potentials in clinical diagnosis. boston: butterworth. timon, c.i., & walsh, m.a.' (1989). sudden hearing loss as a presentation of hiv infection. journal of laryngology and otology, 103, 1071-1072. tshabalala-msimang, m.e. (1999). overview of the national hiv sero-prevalence survey of women attending public antenatal clinics in south africa: national hiv/aids statistics/ 13th international aids conference, south african department of health, rsa. wiley, t.l., & fowler, c.g. (1997). acoustic immittance measures in clinical audiology. san diego: singular publishing. wilson, j.w., jones, b., & fridjhon, p. (1998). use of the nal-ab wordlists as a south african english speech discrimination test. the south african journal of communication disorders, 45. die sid-afrikaansetydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings vol. 49, 2002 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the production of coherent narrative texts by older language impaired children. s h a r o n t u c h , β a . (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) speech therapy department, transvaal memorial hospital for children, johannesburg. summary a group of 4 language-impaired children, 9 years old, and a group of 4 control children with n o language problems were compared on an aspect of 'communicative competence' their ability t o produce coherent narrative texts (sequences of sentences) which were semantically coherent and appropriate to the situational context. a test was devised by t h e writer, comprising stories presented t o the children through a number of sensory modalities. the narrative texts elicited from the 2 groups were compared on a number of measures of semantic cohesion and measures of general semantic content (or appropriateness t o t h e situational c o n t e x t ) . the performance of the languageimpaired children appeared t o be inferior to the control group on all the measures of semantic cohesion and general semantic c o n t e n t , supporting the hypothesis that the language-impaired group would perform inferiorly t o the control group on an aspect of 'communicative competence'. the implications of t h e study's findings for the diagnosis and t r e a t m e n t of expressive language problems in the older child were discussed. opsomming 'n groep van 4 9-jarige kinders met taalprobleme is vergelyk met 'n kontrole-groep van 4 kinders ten opsigte van 'n aspek van 'kommunikatiewe vermoe'. ten einde hierdie aspek te ondersoek is hulle vermoe om saamgestelde verhalende paragrawe te produseer, ontleed. hierdie paragrawe moes ook toepaslik met betrekking t o t die situasie wees. die ondersoeker het vervolgens 'n toets saamgestel, bestaande uit stories wat deur verskeie sensoriese modaliteite aan die kinders meegedeel is. die verkree response van beide groepe is vergelyk ten opsigte van 'n aantal maatstawwe van semantiese samehang en maatstawwe van algemene semantiese inhoud (of toepaslikheid t o t situasieleidrade). resultate verkry uit evaluasie van semantiese samehang en semantiese inhoud het getoon dat die groep van kinders met taalprobleme swakker presteer het as die kontrole-groep. hierdeur word die hipotese, dat kinders met taalprobleme ondergeskik is aan kinders met normale taalontwikkeling ten opsigte van 'kommunikatiewe vermoe', dus gestaaf. die waarde van bevindinge verkry uit die studie vir die diagnose en behandeling van ekspressiewe taalprobleme in die ouer kind, word vervolgens bespreek. linguistics has had an increasing influence on work in language pathology over recent years. currently, diagnosis and treatment of expressive (and receptive) language problems of the child during the initial years of,language acquisition as well as the older child (beyond the first 5 years of language acquisition) has been conducted largely within the linguistic framework of 'grammatical competence' with its emphasis on the grammaticality of single sentences. however, there appears to have been a growing feeling amongst language clinicians that this approach towards diagnosis and treatment of expressive language disorders might not be adequate for expressive language problems in the older child. whereas the younger child is largely expected to be only 'grammatically competent', the older child should be able to produce the south africa journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 43 not only single grammatical sentences but coherent sequences of sentences if he wishes to communicate effectively with others in his environment. this ability appears to require both greater linguistic and cognitive skill and might be deficient in older children, who, having passed the period of early language acquisition, may by this time have acquired 'grammatical competence' at the single sentence level. recent developments in modern linguistic theory appear to offer both a conceptual framework as well as a method of analysis with which to evaluate such an ability and to remediate possible deficits in this ability. growing dissatisfaction expressed by a number of linguists 1 0 , 1 9 with modern linguistic theory's notion of 'grammatical competence' has led to the introduction of (1) a new conceptualisation of 'competence' — 'communicative competence' as well as (2) a method of analysis with which to investigate this new conceptualisation of 'competence' 'discourse analysis'. each of these will be considered briefly. modern linguistic theory, largely based on the generative model of language put forward by chomsky,4 has recognised that the world of modern linguistics comprises 2 parts — 'linguistic competence' and 'linguistic performance'. 'linguistic performance' has been conceptualised as the native speakerlistener's actual production and understanding of an indefinite number of isolated context-less grammatical sentences, generally never spoken nor heard before. 'linguistic competence' has been conceptualised as the set of rules which characterizes the native speaker-listener's abstract, underlying knowledge of his language which permits the 'linguistic performance' discussed above.17 modern linguistic theory until recently has aimed to concern itself exclusively with the formulation of the rules of this particular conceptualisation of 'competence' which will hence be termed 'grammatical competence'.13 criticism has been levelled by certain linguists10·19against linguistic theory's notion of 'grammatical competence' as this characterization of 'competence' makes possible 'performance' which does not approximate performance in actual speaking situations. in order to communicate effectively with others in his environment, it does not suffice for the listener-speaker to produce (and understand) an indefinite number of isolated sentences. in addition, he must be able to produce (and understand) connected discourse defined by harris8 as. . . sentences spoken or written in succession by one or more persons in a single situation, these sentences being appropriate to their situational context. in order to account for this ability to produce sentences appropriate to their verbal and situational context, certain linguists1 0·1 9 have recognised the need to supplement the restricted notion of 'grammatical competence' with an additional conceptualisation of 'competence' 'communicative competence' and 'performance' has been reconceptualised as the actual application of 'communicative' and 'grammatical competence' in real-life speaking situations.20 as 'communicative competence' involves the ability to produce sentences appropriate to their verbal and situational context, the data used for analysis can no longer be the corpus of isolated context-less sentences used for the investigation of 'grammatical competence'. the data used must necessarily be die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 sharon tuch 'connected discourse' as defined above by harris.8 the term designated for this type of analysis is 'discourse analysis'. (the terms 'discourse' and 'text' will be used interchangeably). discourse analysis concerns itself with the .. . isolation of linguistic features which differentiate a coherent (connected) sequence of sentences a text from an agglomerate of sentences.6 the majority of work on text cohesion (relation between sentences) 7 has dealt with the syntactic aspects of intersentence dependence or relation and has considered different syntactic types of intersentence relation. amongst these are the two types of intersentence relation investigated in the present study: (1) the use of 'anaphora', when one element in a sentence refers back to an earlier sentence and (2) the use of conjunctions. however, there has been growing realization that the consideration of semantic, in addition to syntactic, aspects of intersentence relation is essential if the problem of text cohesion is to be successfully resolved.8 gleason 8 in his discussion of semantic cohesion in narrative texts stated that the most important aspect of semantic ('deep' in gleason's terminology) cohesion is the chain of events which 'forms the backbone of the narrative'. gleason's scheme, the 'event line' has been adapted for the semantic analysis in the present study. research into the child's development of this aspect of 'communicative competence' (as well as other aspects of 'communicative competence') has been limited. since 1960, investigations9 1 3 > 1 4 1 s have focused on the development of the structure of the single sentence and did not consider the ability to produce coherent sequences of sentences. piaget,16 despite the fact that he has not worked within a linguistic framework, did investigate in a portion of his work the development of communication skills the child's ability to produce coherent sequences of sentences appropriate to the situation. piaget's work is considered to have relevance for the present study for the following reasons :(1) it has provided a certain amount of information on the development, with age, of the ability to produce sequences of sentences — the progression from 'egocentric' to 'socialised' language. (2) piaget isolated the features of 'egocentric language', a number of which were considered in the evaluation of the ability in this study.6 (3) his work provided a perspective on factors which might account for deficits in this ability.s while a limited amount of research has been done (notably by piaget16 and flavell5) on the development of communication skills, no research to the author's knowledge has been conducted within a linguistic framework into the ability of normal or language-impaired children to produce coherent sequences of sentences appropriate to the situational context an aspect of 'communicative competence.' the present study aimed to investigate this aspect of 'communicative competence' in both normal and language-impaired older children. the study was undertaken with a view to: ' (1) providing a more comprehensive framework for the diagnosis and treatthe south african journal of communication disorders. vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 45 ment of expressive language problems in the older child a framework which would incorporate 'communicative competence' in addition to 'grammatical competence'. (2) suggesting a method of analysis with which to evaluate an aspect of 'communicative competence' 'discourse analysis'. (3) offering a test which might be used to appraise an aspect of 'communicative competence'. methodology aim to compare two groups of 9-year-old children, a language-impaired and a normal group, on an aspect of 'communicative competence' their ability to produce narrative texts which are semantically coherent and appropriate to the situational context. • more specifically the study aims to compare the two groups on: (1) their ability to provide the chain of events which according to gleason 8 'forms the backbone' of the stories presented to them. this involves the appraisal of general semantic content (or appropriateness to the situational context) as well as an appraisal of the most important aspect of semantic cohesion.8 (2) their use of two types of intersentence relation, which contribute to the semantic cohesion within a text. (a) their use of conjunctions which are semantically appropriate as well as their use of conjunctions acquired later in the developmental sequence of language acquisition. (b) their anaphoric use of pronouns with non-ambiguous referents. subjects two groups of subjects (ss) were used, an experimental group (e grp.) and control group (c grp.). each group comprised 4ss, 3 girls and 1 boy. all ss came from monolingual english-speaking home environments. the ε grp. comprised ss diagnosed by a speech-therapist or remedial teacher to have language problems of a predominantly expressive nature. the ss were attending either private remedial schools or 'pilot' (remedial) classes in government schools. the criteria for the selection of the ε grp. were (1) age ε ss were required to be between 8% to 9v< years of age. this particular age range was selected as it was believed, on the basis of work done by piaget,16 and a pilot study carried out by the present author, that children should by this age have acquired the particular aspect of 'communicative competence' investigated in the study. (2) intellectual functioning all children were required to be of average intellectual functioning, judged on the basis of a full scale i.q. assessment range of 100-110. i.q. tests had been carried out recently at the schools. this was considered necessary as c. chomsky3 found that the stage of language acquisition reached by children was related to i.q. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 sharon tuch (3) socio-economic level all ε ss appeared to be from middle-class backgrounds in terms of the areas where they lived and the occupational status of their fathers (all white-collar workers — professional or in important business positions e.g. managers or directors). it was considered necessary to use ss from middle-class backgrounds in view of the work done by bernstein1· on the effect of lower-class backgrounds on children's language. it was originally required that ε ss had no visual perceptual difficulties. this was considered to be of importance as a portion of the test used by the writer involved picture interpretation. however, it was not possible to obtain ss with exclusively language problems. all ε ss were reported in their remedial assessments to have visual perceptual difficulties largely with regard to their perception of form rather than pictures per se. it was recognized that, if their performance on the portion of the test involving picture interpretation was inadequate, the influence of visual perceptual difficulties rather than linguistic factors could not be ruled out. three of the four ss had received speech therapy, two of whom were still receiving speech therapy. the fourth ε s had only recently been diagnosed to have a language problem. the varied amount of speech therapy received by the ε ss did not appear to be a critical factor as all the ε ss were still considered to have language problems. the c grp. comprised ss reported by their teachers to have no language problems or problems of any nature. these ss were all attending the same government school. the following variables were considered in the matching of the c and ε grps:(1) age; (2) sex; (3) intellectual functioning; (4) socio-economic background. test used in view of the unsuitability of available tests to assess children's ability to produce narrative texts, the present author devised her own test, following a pilot study which cannot be elaborated on in the present article. the test comprised:— (1) 4 different picture sequences (of 6 pictures each) each relating a story. (2) 2 verbal stories. the stories were presented in three different ways, distinguished in terms of the sensory modality of input or combination thereof. each presentation of a story was followed by the elicitation of a narrative text from the child about the story. the three types of stofy presentation were: (1) input through the visual modality two picture sequences were presented to the child. ; (2) input through the auditory modality another two stories were recounted verbally (auditorily) to the child. (3) input through a combination of the visual and auditory modalities — a further two picture sequences were presented to the child, accompanied the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 47 by a verbal description of the sequence of events depicted in the picture sequence. the reasons for varying the nature of the input in the presentation of the stories were the following: (1) it was postulated that the demands imposed by the different modalities were not entirely the same. an investigation of the effect of modality on performance was considered of value. (2) information about events contained in a narrative text is originally received through a number of possible modalities. it was thus believed that stories presented through different modalities or combinations thereof would elicit discourse which was more representative of real-life spoken discourse than the presentation of stories through a single modality. all stories were intended to be of a comparable nature in view of the writer's intention to determine the effect of different modalities on performance without the interference of the variable of story content. the following factors were considered:(1) the nature of the content (stories depicted every-day events). (2) the number of main events per story. (3) the nature of the relationship between main events (causal or temporal). (4) the number of pictures per picture sequence. (5) the number of sentences and the nature of the syntax of the sentences in the auditory stories. pictures were designed to be as clear and non-ambiguous as possible. factors considered by the writer to ensure the clarity of the pictures were (1) picture size (2) background the reduction of distracting background detail and (3) realism. "thus any possible pictorial misinterpretation by the children could not be attributed to the lack of clarity of the pictures. procedure (1) testing of each of the eight ss was conducted individually during a single 30-minute session under quiet conditions with only the tester (the writer) and the subject present in the room. (2) instructions were provided and a trial picture story was presented to the child (not analysed) in order to ascertain that the child "understood the task required of him and to familiarise him with the test . situation. (3) the tester then presented the 6 stories, each followed by the elicitation of a narrative text about the particular story presented. the stories used in a particular modality were the same for each child. (4) the order of modality presentation was varied systematically for each child to eliminate a practice effect. (5) the child was given as much time as he required before telling the story in order to elicit narrative texts to the best of the child's ability. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24,1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 sharon tuch (6) all four auditory stories were pre-recorded on a tape recorder to ensure uniformity in the presentation of stories. (7) the narrative texts of the children were recorded on a second taperecorder. (8) though six narrative texts (two per modality) were elicited from each child, three narrative texts (one per modality) were analysed for each child in view of time limitations. the three particular texts analysed were selected randomly but were the same for all ss. (9) the three narrative texts, were analysed and scored in terms of a number of measures and the scores for the two groups on these measures were compared (see measures below). the influence of modality was investigated only for measures of the ability to provide the chain of events: it was believed that the performance on this measure might be most affected by different sensory modalities. (10) a descriptive rather than inferential statistical analysis of the results was carried out: the results were not considered to lend themselves to a reliable inferential statistical analysis in view of the 'open-ended' responses of the children. measures u s e d a n d their scoring the following measures were used: i. measures of the ability to provide the chain of events which 'forms the backbone of the narrative' stories presented to the children. these measures were suggested by gleason's concept of the 'event line'. measures of this ability were divided into two categories:(a) measures of the ability to provide the general semantic content of the story presented, i.e. measures of the ability to respond appropriately to the situational context the story-stimuli presented. (b) measures of the ability to provide a semantically (or logically) coherent narrative text. only those deviations in content which affected the semantic cohesion of the narrative text were recorded here. it was necessary to distinguish these two categories as deviations in content affecting not only semantic accuracy but also, the logic of the story resulted in a greater disturbance of meaning and appeared to suggest possible difficulties at the higher cognitive level of logical thinking or'reasoning. the three stories presented to the children were each broken down into a number of main events, the omission of which would alter either the content or both the content and semantic cohesion of the text. (children were not required to provide the prpcise wording of the main events outlined but to convey rather the 'idea' of these events.) the three narrative texts of each child were analysed in terms of the following: (a) measures of content consisted of the total number of (1) main events omitted per story, and (2) number of inaccuracies concerning the main events per story. it was considered necessary to include this latter measure as children, on occasions provided a main event with an inaccuracy concerning a particular aspect of the event, which altered to the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 49 some extent the content of the story. (3) the number of additional events provided per story were noted. these events described very concrete aspects of the situation portrayed in the story and were of no relevance to the story, they tended to obscure the main events of the story. (b) measures of semantic (or logical) cohesion consisted of (1) the number of main events omitted, affecting story logic per story, (2) the number of inaccuracies affecting logic per story, and (3) the frequency of incorrect verbal sequencing of main events per story. these instances of incorrect sequencing disturbed the semantic cohesion of the text. scoring of the above measures: for each child: for each of the measures (a) (1), (a) (2), (b) (1) and (b) (2) a percentage was established for each story and a mean percentage for all three stories was computed. for measures (a) (3) and (b) (3) a frequency score was established (e.g. no. of additional events) for each story and then a mean frequency score was tallied. for each group: for each of measure (a) (1), (a) (2), (b) (1) and (b) (2) a group mean percentage was calculated on each story modality and then a grand group mean percentage for all three modalities together was established. for each of measures (a) (3) and (b) (3) a group mean frequency was tallied for each story modality and then a grand group mean frequency for all three modalities together was established. grand group means for all six measures were compared for all the modalities together. group means for all six measures were compared on each modality. ii. measures of two types of intersentence relation contributing to the semantic cohesion of the text: (a) conjunctions, and (b) personal pronouns with non-ambiguous referents were used. both types of intersentence relation contribute to the syntactic in addition to the semantic cohesion within the text. however in the present study only the semantic aspects of intersentence relation were considered. (a) the use of conjunctions to link sentences within the text: conjunctions were divided into two main categories: — conjunctions which manifest themselves early in the developmental sequence of language acquisition 'and', 'then' and 'so' used with the semantic intent of 'then' rather than 'therefore'. — conjunctions which manifest themselves later in the developmental sequence causal conjunctions ('because' and 'so' used to denote 'therefore'), time conjunctions ('when' and 'until') and other later-developing conjunctions ('but', ' i f ' , ' in case', 'in order to'). 1 2 other later developing conjunctions could not be considered in the study as the nature of the stories was not conducive to their use. the following two measures were used in the analysis of children's use of conjunctions: (1) number of later developing conjunctions used semantically appropriately. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 sharon tuch this measure-suggested by flavell5 was considered of importance as the laterdeveloping conjunctions express more complex relations (than early-developing conjunctions) between the sentences which they link. their use within a text would thus enhance the semantic cohesion of the text. it was postulated on the basis of the work of flavell5 and piaget1 6 that the ε grp. would use a smaller percentage of these later developing conjunctions semantically appropriately. (2) number of conjunctions used semantically inappropriately. this measure was considered necessary as a failure to observe the semantic restrictions governing the use of particular conjunctions would lead to a disruption of the semantic cohesion (or continuity) within the text. it was postulated on the basis of lee's w o r k 1 2 that the ε grp. would use a higher proportion of conjunctions in a semantically inappropriate manner than the c grp· scoring of the two measures of conjunction use. a percentage for each of the two measures was established for each child on the basis of his three narrative texts. group mean percentages on each measure were obtained and compared. (b) the use of pronouns with non-ambiguous referent. the second type of intersentence relation considered was the anaphoric use of personal pronouns i.e. personal pronouns said to substitute for an earlier occurring noun phrase (np) in a preceding sentence of the text. the writer chose to investigate an aspect of anaphoric pronoun use which was of importance for semantic cohesion the anaphoric use of personal pronouns with non-ambiguous referents (a measure suggested by piaget1 6). this refers to the use of a pronoun only when the earlier occurring np, to which it is intended to refer, is not to be confused with other earlier occurring nps. ambiguous anaphoric use of personal pronouns is more likely to occur when there are two or more human participants in the text of the same sex; or when there are two or more non-human nouns in the text, both singular and plural. the following was investigated: (1) ambiguous use of human personal pronouns ('he', 'him', 'his' or 'she', 'her') in two of the three stories analysed per child; (2) ambiguous use of non-human personal pronouns ('it' or 'them') in two of the three stories analysed. it was postulated on the basis of work done by piaget1 6 and flavell5 that the ε grp. would use a higher proportion of personal pronouns ambiguously than the c grp. scoring of ambiguous pronominal use. each child's stories were scored in terms of1 ι (1) number of ambiguous human personal pronouns in two stories, (2) number of ambiguous non-human personal pronouns in two stories, and the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 24,1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 51 (3) number of ambiguous personal pronouns,which was computed as a percentage on the basis of scores (1) and (2). mean group percentages for (3) were then obtained and compared. results and discussion. before presenting the results it should be pointed out that conclusions about the significance of differences between the two groups could not be drawn as an inferential statistical analysis was not carried out. it must also be mentioned that, though included in certain tables, consideration of the performance of individual ss was not possible in this present article. results, for each measure will be presented and discussed. i. measures of the ability to provide the chain o f e v e n t s o f the s t o r y . a comparison of the two groups' performance on measures of semantic content and semantic cohesion — all modalities being considered together. while the disparity between the two groups was small on certain measures, scores for the two groups appeared to indicate that (1) the ε grp. performed inferiorly to the c grp. on all measures of semantic content resulting in their texts being less appropriate to the situational context (or stories presented.) (2) the ε grp. performed inferiorly to the c grp. on all measures of semantic cohesion, the presence of these deviations in semantic cohesion casting greater doubt on the higher cognitive processes of reasoning or logical thought of these children. the two groups performance will be compared firstly in terms of (a) measures of semantic content and then in terms of (b) measures of semantic cohesion (see table 1). (a) measures of content: (1) the grand mean percentage of events omitted by the ε grp. of the total number of events per story was 13,6» for the ε grp as compared with 2,5% for the c grp. the ε grp thus showed a tendency to omit more events of a story than the c grp. (2) the grand mean percentage of inaccuracies concerning the main events of the total number of events per story was 9,5% for the ε grp. and 2,3% for the c grp. these results appear to indicate a greater tendency in the ε grp to provide events with inaccuracy. (3) the mean frequency of additional events per story was 0.5 for the ε grp. and 0 for the c grp. the disparity between the two groups was small but considered noteworthy in view of the presence of these events in the ε grp's texts as compared with their total absence in the c grp's texts. the presence of these events in the texts of the ε grp. seems to indicate that the ε grp. tended to be more 'concrete-bound' than the c grp and experienced difficulty on occasions in extracting the main (salient) events of the story from the concrete background occurrences within the story (a more abstract cognitive function). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 sharon tuch measures visual auditory visual & auditory all modalities together ε c ε c ε c ε c percentage of events omitted of the total no. of events per story 14% 16% 4% 11% 3,5% 13,6% 2,5% percentage of inaccuracies about events of the total no. of events per story 25% 7% 3,5% 9,5% 2,3% frequency of additional events per story ,75 ,25 ,33 . percentage of events omitted affecting semantic cohesion of the total no. of events per story 756 16% 1% 3,5% 10% 1,2% percentage of inaccuracies affecting semantic cohesion of the total no. of events per story 18% 6% frequency of incorrect sequencing per story ,5 ,25 ,25 table 1 ε and c group means on semantic measures of content and cohesion by modality and all the modalities together. (b) measures of semantic (or logical) cohesion: (1) the grand mean percentage of omissions of events affecting logical cohesion of the total number of main events per story was 10% for the ε grp. and 1,2% for the c grp. the ε grp thus omitted a greater proportion of events affecting the logical cohesion of their stories. (2) the grand mean percentages of inaccuracies affecting the semantic cohesion of the story of the total number of events per story was 6% for the ε grp as compared with 0% for the c grp. while the disparity in percentage between the two groups on this measure was small, it did appear that the ε grp tended to provide a greater proportion of inaccuracies affecting the logical cohesion of their stories as compared with the c grp. (3) the grand mean frequency of incorrect sequence per text was 0,25 for the ε grp and 0 for the c grp. the disparity in frequency between the two groups was again small; however, it was considered noteworthy that there was a total absence of incorrect verbal sequencing in the c grp, whereas there were a number of these instances in the ε grp's stories. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 53 the effect of modality. an in-depth comparison of the performance of the groups on the different story modalities was not made in view of the limited number of texts (one text) per modality for each s. on the basis of the brief comparison made, the reliability of which is recognised to be uncertain, it was apparent that the ε grp performed inferiorly to the c grp on each modality. this appears to indicate that the inferior semantic performance of the ε grp as a whole was not modality dependent. it was, however, of interest that the largest differences between the two groups occurred in the visual modality where the ε grp performed inferiorly to the c grp on all the semantic measures used for the analysis. this appeared attributable in part to the visual perceptual difficulties which the children reportedly experienced. in addition, the interpretive (perceptual-cognitive) demands appeared to be greater in this modality as compared with the audi-, tory modality and the combination of the visual and auditory modalities where the salient events had already been extracted for the child in the verbal (auditory) presentation. the finding appears to have important implications for the language therapist working with language-impaired children the therapist should consider the possibility of visual in addition to language problems with such children. factors postulated to account for the inferior performance of the ε grp on the above semantic measures. (1) difficulty in the gaining of meaning from the different modalities of input — visual and auditory: in order to gain meaning from these modalities, the following skills are necessary (a) perceptual skills (auditory and visual), (b) verbal comprehension (for auditory modality) and (c) cognitive skills the ability to extract only the salient (main) events of the story and the logical and temporal relations between these events. the ε grp may have experienced difficulty in the gaining of meaning from the modalities of presentation due to deficits at one or a number of these levels which appear to be interrelated aspects of cognition. piaget16 attributed deficits in perceptual and cognitive skills (which appeared to be reflected in some of the children's misinterpretation and distortion of events) to the child's generalised cognitive 'egocentrism' his being a 'prisoner of his own point of view.' (2) difficulty in expressing meaning which the child has in fact grasped: it is possible that in certain instances the ε ss were able to grasp the salient events of the story and the relations between them, but had difficulty in providing the events accurately and in the correct sequence due to expressive language difficulties involving syntax and/or vocabulary. in addition, piaget16 maintains that the child up to 7—8 years of age tends to omit events and provide them in the incorrect sequence due to his failure to consider the listener's point of view ('egocentric language') which piaget considered to be a further manifestation of the child's generalised cognitive 'egocentrism', his functioning at a less mature cognitive level. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 sharon tuch it is postulated by the writer that an interaction of both the above groups of factors (linguistic and cognitive) might have accounted for the inferior performance of the ε grp on the above semantic measures (of content and cohesion). it is recognised that the relative contribution of these factors may have varied for individual ε ss. ii measures of the use of 2 types of intersentence relation (a) use of conjunctions. (1) the mean percentage of later developing conjunctions used semantically appropriately of the total number of conjunctions used was 20,5% for the ε grp and 33% for the c grp. (see table ii). the ε grp thus used a lower mean percentage of later developing conjunctions semantically appropriately as compared with the c grp. as the later developing conjunctions establish more com. plex and intimate relationships between the sentences of a text, the semantic cohesion of the ε grp's texts thus appeared to be somewhat reduced in relation to the cohesion of the c grp's texts. two factors, one or both of which (as in the case of semantic measures of content and cohesion) may be operative, are postulated to account for the lower percentage of later developing conjunctions found in the ε grp: (i) a cognitive factor piaget 1 6 maintains that the use of causal and logical conjunctions (the majority of later developing conjunctions being logical and causal) in children's spontaneous language develops as children begin to grasp the actual causal and logical relationships expressed by these conjunctions. the lack of causal and logical conjunctions was considered by piaget to be a feature of the child's 'egocentric language'.6it is possible that the ε grp had not mastered to the same extent as the c grp these logical and causal relationships and were thus cognitively delayed in relation to the c grp. (ii) linguistic factors — it is possible that the ε grp did have a non-linguistic (or cognitive) understanding of these causal and logical relationships but did not have the linguistic means (vocabulary of conjunctions and/or syntactic ability) to express these relationships. it would appear that the process of subordination, involved in the use of most later-developing conjunctions,12 imposes greater syntactic demands on the child as subordination involves the linking of units which are constituents at different levels, whereas co-ordination (involved in the use of early conjunctions) involves the linking of units which are constituents at the same level. it is believed that future investigation of the use of later-developing conjunctions may be carried out even more effectively using stories which provide greater scope for the utilisation of these conjunctions. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 55 ss *see across el 44 e2 16 e3 22 e4 0 χ egrp 20,5 ci 40 c2 20 c3 36 c4 35 x c grp 33 * later-developing conjunctions used semantically appropriately as a percentage of the total no. of conjunctions (early and late-developing) used. table ii later developing conjunctions used semantically appropriately as a percentage of the total number of conjunctions used by s and group (2) the percentage of conjunctions used semantically inappropriately of the total number of conjunctions used was 10% for the ε grp and 1% for the c grp (see table iii). while it is difficult to comment on the magnitude of the differences between the 2 groups, it is apparent that the ε grp used a higher proportion of conjunctions in a semantically inappropriate manner as compared with the c grp. as semantic errors in conjunction use disrupt the semantic cohesion or continuity of the text, the higher proportion of semantically inappropriate conjunctions in the ε grp. was responsible for reduced semantic cohesion within their texts. semantic errors in conjunction use in the ε grp. occurred for both earlier and later-developing conjunctions and would reflect the ε grp's failure on occasions to observe the semantic restrictions governing the use of these conjunctions e.g. the conjunction 'then' can be used to link two sentences only when the action described in the sentence preceding the conjunction is complete (not ongoing) prior to the action contained in the sentence following the conjunction. this finding of greater semantic inappropriateness appeared to be in agreement with l e e 1 2 who die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 sharon tuch ss percentage inappropriate el 8% e2 0% e3 22% e4 10% xegrp 10% ci 0% ' c2 5% c3 0% c4 0% xc grp 1,25% table iii conjunctions used semantically inappropriately as a percentage of the total number of conjunctions used by s and group. reports that. . . some children in clinical language training show difficulty learning the semantic constraints of conjunction selection and will make inappropriate choices. β a measure of the anaphoric use of personal pronouns with non-ambiguous referents:the mean group percentage of pronouns used ambiguously (i.e. with two possible referents) of the total number of personal pronouns used anaphorically in the texts investigated was 13,5% for the ε grp and 1% for the c grp. it is evident that the ε grp used a greater percentage of pronouns ambiguously as compared with the c grp. the greater occurrence of ambiguous pronominal usage in the ε grp reduced to some extent the semantic cohesion of their texts as well as their communicative efficacy as compared with'the c grp. again two factors, one or both of which may be operative, are postulated to account for this greater tendency in the ε grp. to use pronouns with ambiguous referents: ' a cognitive factor piaget16 maintains that the use of pronouns with non-explicit referents is a feature of 'egocentric' language. according to piaget, 'egocentric' language declines as a function of the child's the south african journal of communication disorders. vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 57 cognitive development and is replaced by 'socialised' language. it is thus possible that the ε grp, at 9 years of age, were still using pronouns with ambiguous .referents in view of cognitive delay. a linguistic factor it is possible that the ε grp do consider the listener's point of view (and are thus not cognitively delayed in this regard) but are unable to express this consideration in view of other more important linguistic and cognitive demands imposed by the discourse situation. flavell5 recognised that seemingly 'egocentric' language might not be attributable to a failure to consider the listener's point of view per se but rather to deficits in linguistic skills. further investigation of ambiguous pronominal use may be enhanced by consideration of the following points: (1) demands on the ability to use pronouns with non-ambiguous referents may be increased by the use of stories involving more than two participants of the same sex or more than two non-human nouns (singular or plural). (2) the use of auditory stories or visual stimuli no longer present in the testing situation is likely to increase the speaker's need to be explicit in his use of pronouns the listener is less able to extract the appropriate referent in the absence of pictures. general discussion and conclusion in view of the few deviations on all measures it appeared that the normal 9-year-old group were 'communicatively competent' and that the languageimpaired grp of the same age failed to exhibit the 'communicative competence' required for their age level. the ε grp's inferior performance on the measures used to investigate an aspect of 'communicative competence' appeared to be most comprehensively accounted for by the perspective furnished by piaget,16 and flavell:5 an interaction between both linguistic (semantic and syntactic) and cognitive deficits, the relative contribution of which may have varied for individual ss. the findings of the study would suggest that at least certain language impaired children exhibit deficits on an aspect of 'communicative competence' which impair their communication with others in their environment. while the value of diagnosis and treatment of expressive problems in terms of 'grammatical competence' is recognised, it is believed that this approach needs to be supplemented with the concept of 'communicative competence' for the following reason: an approach to expressive language problems in the older child in terms of 'grammatical competence' alone might not be able to detect and hence remediate deficits beyond the single sentence level. thus it cannot ensure the child's communicative efficacy, effective communication being to some extent dependent on the ability to produce coherent sequences of sentences. furthermore, such an approach might lead to premature dismissal of children from language therapy as rehabilitated in terms of 'grammatical competence', when, in fact, they still have difficulty in terms of 'communicative competence' which warrants attention. it is further believed that such an approach could also be utilised in the appraisal of aphasic language. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 sharon tuch the present study appears to have a further implication for the approach to expressive language problems in the older (and possibly younger) language-impaired child. the study focused largely on semantic ability (the ability to express meaning) as this ability appears to be crucial for effective communication or 'communicative competence'. all the language-impaired ss investigated showed deficits in this ability. despite the report that the ε ss were predominantly expressively impaired, the difficulty in their expression of meaning appeared to be the result, not only of linguistic deficits, but possibly of the interaction between linguistic and perceptual-cognitive deficits. currently, diagnosis and therapy for expressive language problems focuses largely on linguistic deficits involving syntax and to some extent vocabulary, which only partially accounts for the ability to express meaning. the findings of the study appear to suggest the need for (a) greater emphasis on the ability to express meaning and (b) a more global approach considering both linguistic and cognitive factors even in the diagnosis and treatment of what seem to present as predominantly expressive language problems. discourse analysis appeared to be a necessary tool for the investigation of the particular aspect of 'communicative competence' considered in the study; it facilitates the examination of aspects of semantic cohesion which could not be investigated at the single sentence level, except possibly for the use of conjunctions which was nevertheless considered to be more profitably investigated in discourse where there is scope for the conjoining of a greater number of sentences. a consideration of the child's discourse rather than single sentences, lends itself also to the investigation of other features which are of importance for effective communication, though not included in the present study: (a) syntactic cohesion established through a number of types of intersentence relation, e.g. ellipsis, tense continuity and the anaphoric use of the determiner 'the' and pro-forms (e.g. pro-verbs, pro-adjective); (b) the ability to produce grammatical sentences in discourse rather than at the isolated sentence level; (c) fluency in a discourse situation. the test devised was considered to be of value as it enabled an appraisal of the aspect of 'communicative competence* investigated in the study and proved to be sensitive to deficits in this aspect of 'communicative competence'. it is recognised, however, that a more complex test of a similar nature might have been more appropriate for the age level investigated and revealed even greater differences between the two groups. this can be ascertained only with future research. the test also provided a controlled situation for the investigation of an aspect of 'communicative competence': the content of the children's narrative texts could be evaluated against the content of the stories presented. the test makes it possible to establish the influence of modality of input on this aspect of 'communicative competence' which could have both diagnostic and therapeutic value. the test also enabled an appraisal of what the writer considers to be 'integrated linguistic functioning' — the ability to gain meaning and then express that meaning, which requires substantial cognitive and linguistic integrity. this integrated linguistic functioning would appear to be the hallmark of 'communicative competence'. finally, it is believed that the test (or picture and auditory stories the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) coherent narrative texts by older children 59 of a similar nature) could be utilised therapeutically to promote the development of both cognitive and linguistic skill. in conclusion, further research employing larger samples of ss and a greater number of texts per s appears advocated to investigate the aspect of 'communicative competence' as well as its development with age. as the judgement of 'communicative competence' is ultimately the listener's, the ratings of other speech therapists and untrained listener's of children's 'communicative competence' should be obtained. also, children should be required to provide information to a listener who is ignorant of this information: this would most likely increase the need on their part to communicate effectively and hence enable a more accurate appraisal of this ability. references 1. bernstein, b. (1962): social class, linguistic codes, and grammatical elements. language & speech, 5,221-240. 2. brown, r. (1973): a first language the early stages. george allen & unwin, london. 3. chomsky, c. (1970): stages in language development and reading exposure . harvard educational review, 42 (1), 1 -33. 4. chomsky, n. (1973): current issues in linguistic theory. mouton, the hague. 5. flavell, j. h. (1968): the development of role-taking and communication skills in children. wiley, new york. 6. ginsburg, h. & opper, s. (1969): piaget's theory of intellectual development. prentice hall, new jersey. 7. hasan, r. (1968): grammatical cohesion in spoken and written english, i. (programmes in linguistics and english teaching, paper 7) longman, london. 8. hendricks, w. 0 . (1973): essays on semiolinguistics and verbal art. mouton, the hague. 9. hunt, k. w. (1970): syntactic maturity in school children and adults. monographs of the society for research in child development. 35(1), serial no. 134. 10. hymes, d. (1972): on communicative competence. in sociolinguistics. pride, j. b. & holmes, j. (eds). penguin. 11. johnson, d. j. & myklebust, h. r. (1964): learning disabilities. grune & stratton, inc., new york. 12. lee, l. (1974): developmental sentence analysis. northwestern university press, evanston. 13. lyons, j. (1973): introduction. in new horizons in linguistics, lyons, j. (ed). penguin, middlesex. 14. menyuk, p. (1964): syntactic rules used by children from preschool through first grade. child development, 35,533-546. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sharon tuch 6 0 15 o'donnell, r. c., griffin, w. j. & norris, r. c. (1967): syntax of kindergarten and elementary school children: a transformational analysis. national council of teachers of english, report no. 8. champaign, illinois. 16. piaget, j. (1926): the language and thought of the child. routledge & kegan paul ltd., london. 17. pit corder, s. (1973): introducing applied linguistics. penguin education, middlesex. 18. saling, m. (1976): personal communication. senior lecturer, department of psychology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. 19. wales, r. & campbell, r. (1973): the study of language acquisition. in new horizons in linguistics, lyons, j. (ed.), penguin, middlesex. 20. young, d. n. (1971): a sociolinguistic analysis of some aspects of discourse structure in newspaper texts with proposals for pedagogic application. master's dissertation, department of linguistics, university of edinburgh. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abstract introduction research method and design results ethical considerations discussion conclusion acknowledgements references about the author(s) esedra krüger department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa alta kritzinger department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa lidia pottas department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa citation krüger, e., kritzinger, a., & pottas, l. (2017). breastfeeding and swallowing in a neonate with mild hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy. south african journal of communication disorders 64(1), a209. https://doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v64i1.209 original research breastfeeding and swallowing in a neonate with mild hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy esedra krüger, alta kritzinger, lidia pottas received: 10 oct. 2016; accepted: 20 mar. 2017; published: 22 may 2017 copyright: © 2017. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract background: specific breastfeeding and swallowing characteristics in neonates with hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy (hie) have not yet been well described in the literature. considering the relatively high incidence of hie in resource-poor settings, speech-language therapists should be cognisant of the feeding difficulties in this population during breastfeeding. objective: to systematically describe the breastfeeding and swallowing of a single case of a neonate diagnosed with mild hie from admission to discharge. method: a case study of a 2-day old neonate with mild hie in a neonatal intensive care unit at an urban teaching hospital, is presented. data were prospectively collected during four sessions in a 12-day period until the participant’s discharge. feeding and swallowing were assessed clinically, as well as instrumentally using a video-fluoroscopic swallow study. results: after parenteral feeding, nasogastric tube feeding commenced. breastfeeding was introduced on day 6, as it was considered a safe option, and revealed problematic rooting, shallow latching, short sucking bursts, infrequent swallowing, and a drowsy state of arousal, with coughing and choking. no penetration or aspiration was identified instrumentally. after 13 days, the neonate was breastfeeding safely. conclusion: although the pharyngeal stage of swallowing was intact, symptoms of oral stage dysphagia were revealed using a combination of clinical and instrumental measures. breastfeeding difficulties were identified, exacerbated by poor state regulation, which lead to prolonged hospitalisation. the case study highlights the unexpected long duration of feeding difficulties in an infant with mild hie and indicates further research. introduction hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy (hie) describes abnormal neurologic behaviour in the neonate because of a hypoxic-ischaemic event (groenendaal & de vries, 2015). organ dysfunction, specifically of the heart, lungs, gut, kidneys and the brain, is associated with hie (thyagarajan et al., 2015). hie is referred to as a possible neurological cause of dysphagia (dodrill & gosa, 2015; garg, 2004; rybak, 2015). according to sarnat and sarnat observations (1976), as well as other hie grading systems such as the hie score (thompson, van der elst, molteno & malan, 1997) or the modified sarnat encephalopathy grading (horn, 2013; shalak, laptook, velaphi & perlman, 2003), infants with hie may be at risk of early feeding problems. this may be because of a weak or absent rooting and sucking reflex, which is distinctive of all three hie stages [mild (i), moderate (ii) and severe (iii)] (horn, 2013; shalak et al., 2003). infants with neurological conditions may have significant feeding difficulties (genna, levan fram & sandora, 2013). according to tamminen, verronen, saarikoski, göransson and tuomiranta (1983), infants with neonatal encephalopathy because of hypoxia-ischemia, or birth asphyxia as it was previously named, may show breastfeeding difficulties. inadequate muscular control in the infant’s oral area may be related to neurological deficits such as asphyxia (wolf & glass, 1992). a condition such as hie may also impact the infant’s ability to achieve a correct latch-on for breastfeeding (walker, 2017). neurologic impairment in infants may furthermore inhibit state regulation, which may negatively influence an infant’s ability to feed successfully (wolf & glass, 1992). furthermore, gastrointestinal tract impairment, and specifically necrotising enterocolitis (nec), is also a concern following hie (thyagarajan et al., 2015), which may further complicate feeding in this population. infants with neurological impairment are likely to be more disorganised than neuro-typical infants as they are influenced by hospital procedures, medication, and are often separated from their mothers, which may affect their ability to breastfeed (genna et al., 2013). central nervous system involvement such as hie is considered to be a known aetiology of dysphagia in infants (arvedson & brodsky, 2002), but globally, literature describing the specific early breastfeeding and swallowing characteristics in neonates with hie is scarce. limited published data exist on the incidence of hie in sub-saharan africa (horn, 2013). hie has a range of reported incidences, but occurs approximately 1–5 per 1000 live births according to three studies in the united kingdom, australia and sweden (horn, 2013). research in south africa reported incidences of 8.3/1000 and 3.6/1000 (bruckmann & velaphi, 2015; horn, 2013). because of the relatively high incidence of hie in south africa, speech-language therapists (slts) will encounter infants with hie in their caseloads and will be required to be skilled in addressing feeding difficulties that may arise in this population. infants with neurological conditions such as hie may have various issues with feeding including muscle tone disturbances, inadequate coordination, hampered function of certain nerves or neuromuscular units, inappropriate state regulation and limited endurance for feeding (genna et al., 2013). these challenges may influence early breastfeeding, but an infant may adapt over time, depending on the severity of the neurological condition (walker, 2017). the benefits of exclusive breastfeeding for infants have been documented well and are recommended for the first 6 months of life in high-income as well as lowto middle-income countries for all infants (kramer & kakuma, 2001; victoria et al., 2016). breastfeeding challenges that may arise from infants with neurological involvement may include sucking and swallowing difficulties, poor weight gain, separation from the mother in the first week of life, as well as maternal grief and shock at having an infant with a disorder (walker, 2017). the slt being a central team member of an in-patient feeding and swallowing team should address the mentioned issues in affected families (arvedson & brodsky, 2002; walker, 2017). slts, therefore, require an understanding of the profile of the feeding characteristics of neonates with hie in order to render comprehensive services to this population and their families. the following research question was, therefore, posed: what are the early breastfeeding and swallowing characteristics including the feeding difficulties of a neonate with hie in neonatal care? a description of the breastfeeding and swallowing characteristics in a case of hie in a neonate over a period of 12 days is presented. a clinical description may contribute to the dearth of research on the early oral feeding attempts in infants with hie in south africa and abroad. the findings may also assist slts to identify the feeding difficulties in neonates with hie that may lead to longer hospitalisation. research method and design aim the aim of this study was to systematically describe the breastfeeding and swallowing of a neonate diagnosed with hie over a period of 12 days until discharge from the hospital. design a case study was used, and data were collected prospectively. an in-depth description of a single case over time is presented (leedy & ormrod, 2014). setting data were collected in the neonatal intensive care unit (nicu) and high care unit of an urban tertiary hospital in pretoria. neonates diagnosed with hie and deemed suitable candidates, receive whole body therapeutic hypothermia for 72 h. intermittent kangaroo mother care is provided after completion of therapeutic hypothermia. neonates in the nicu and high care unit are provided with neurodevelopmental supportive positioning on warm tables and incubators. the hospital encourages exclusive breastfeeding and expressed breast milk (ebm) by cup as an alternative. participant an inborn post-term male neonate of 2 days old, diagnosed with hie by a paediatrician, and receiving therapeutic hypothermia, was purposefully selected for this study. characteristics of the infant and mother are shown in table 1. table 1: maternal and infant characteristics. according to table 1, the neonate’s highest hie score (thompson et al., 1997) was 4, indicating mild hie, on day 1. no congenital anomalies or metabolic disorders were diagnosed. therapeutic hypothermia is the standard of care for infants with hie globally (tagin, woolcott, vincer, whyte & stinson, 2012). according to the literature, an infant is diagnosed with hie by a medical doctor when the following criteria are met (martinez-biarge, diez-sebastian, wusthoff, mercuri & cowan 2013; volpe, 2012): arterial cord blood gas indicative of metabolic acidosis (ph < 7.1), depressed apgar scores (<7 at 5 and/or 10 min), the presence of neonatal encephalopathy (depression of level of consciousness, usually with respiratory difficulty, abnormal muscle tone, disturbed cranial nerve function especially impaired feeding and often the presence of seizures) and the need for resuscitation. as further indicated in table 1, the neonate was exposed to hiv and received nevirapine, but it was too early to suspect the presence of hiv encephalopathy. the neonate’s mother was of advanced age and received limited antenatal care in comparison with the minimum of four antenatal appointments prescribed for mothers with hiv (national department of health, 2015). mothers who are hiv positive in this hospital setting are counselled about exclusive breastfeeding during antenatal care and at birth as per national department of health (2015) guidelines. both mother and infant receive antiretroviral treatment, and viral loads are monitored to ensure safe breastfeeding. materials well-known literature on paediatric dysphagia and standard parameters of feeding such as coordination of sucking, swallowing and breathing, respiration during feeding and swallowing (arvedson & brodsky, 2002; pereira, sacher, ryan & hayward, 2009; wolf & glass, 1992) were used to compile a data collection sheet and to select relevant information from the hospital file. certain components of the oral motor and feeding assessment checklist (arvedson & brodsky, 2002) were extrapolated for the data collection sheet. this checklist obtains relevant history of the infant and mother, includes a pre-feeding observation of the infant at rest, including a physical examination (such as muscle tone and respiration), as well as observations of non-nutritive sucking and a feeding session at the breast. the hie score (thompson et al., 1997), calculated by the paediatrician, was obtained from the hospital file. the preterm infant breastfeeding behavior scale (pibbs) (nyqvist, rubertsson, ewald & sjöden, 1996) was used to monitor the progress of breastfeeding. the pibbs may be used with full-term infants and was designed as a breastfeeding monitoring tool for use by mothers or clinicians (da costa, van den engel-hoek & bos, 2008; nyqvist, 2013). a video-fluoroscopic swallow study (vfss) was performed using a sysco 19’ version multi diagnosteleva fd (philips, netherlands) screening machine. the contrast liquid used during the vfss was a commercially prepared non-ionic x-ray contrast solution. the contrast liquid was presented in a nuk™ bottle using a narrow nuk™ teat (birth to 6 months) with a small hole. a data collection sheet was compiled for the interpretation of the vfss, which included a checklist compiled from arvedson and brodsky (2002), as well as the penetration aspiration scale (pas) (rosenbek, robbins, roecker, coyle & wood, 1996). the pas is an eight-point classification system to quantify the penetration and aspiration events during vfss (rosenbek et al., 1996). the pas was conceptualised for use with adults, and thus far, data are only available for use in the adult population (arvedson & brodsky, 2002), but has been used in a study on the paediatric population (masarei, wade, stat, mars & sommerlad, 2007). procedure the participant’s feeding was clinically evaluated daily by the first author (esedra krüger), an slt registered with the health professions council of south africa (hpcsa), experienced in assessment of feeding and swallowing in neonates. the assessments were completed during typical feeding sessions in the nicu once the participant was deemed ready for oral feeding by the managing paediatrician. multiple data collection entries were made to allow for capturing of follow-up data. information on the neonate’s medical treatment from admission, as well as his progress, was obtained from the hospital file. the neonate was treated daily, by providing parent guidance and oral stimulation, by a hospital-based slt (arvedson & brodsky, 2002). the pibbs (nyqvist et al., 1996) was completed daily from the commencement of oral feeding on day 6, during direct observation of breastfeeding. direct observation is the least intrusive method of observing breastfeeding while not disrupting the infant’s behaviour (nyqvist et al., 1996). the researcher stood near the mother in a position that provided a clear view of the neonate’s face and chin, according to the description by nyqvist et al. (1996). the neonate was instrumentally assessed on day 13 using vfss, according to recommendations by arvedson and brodsky (2002). the lateral projection view was used, as it yields the most valuable information when studying the paediatric population (arvedson & brodsky, 2002; dematteo, matovic & hjartarson, 2005). the vfss was conducted by two hospital-based slts registered with the hpcsa who has experience in vfss in infants, in conjunction with a radiologist and a radiographer based at the hospital. analysis clinical assessment data were systematically collected by the researcher via observation, without being involved in treatment of the participant (meline, 2010). the clinical assessment data were interpreted using normative data available for full-term infants (arvedson & brodsky, 2002; walker, 2017). the vfss recording, which allowed a frame by frame analysis, was analysed by a panel of two slts experienced in interpretation of vfss in infants. the panel was blinded to the results of the clinical feeding assessments conducted by the researcher. results the results are described chronologically according to the occurrence of events in the participant’s 14-day stay in the hospital and are depicted in figure 1. figure 1: chronological events from admission to discharge. rooting and sucking reflexes were reported to be absent by the paediatrician upon the participant’s admission to the nicu. during the initial clinical assessment on day 2, with a weight of 3.79 kg, the neonate was still receiving whole body therapeutic hypothermia (figure 1). at the scheduled feeding time, he was lethargic, and difficult to rouse, presented with hypotonia, and was unresponsive to stroking on and around the mouth. non-nutritive sucking could not be elicited, although minimal jaw and tongue movement were perceived. no ebm was available yet, and the participant received only parenteral nutrition. the participant received ebm 3-hourly via nasogastric tube (ngt) on the following day after therapeutic hypothermia (day 3). on day 6, the participant was breathing room air, saturating well (>95%) (wolf & glass, 1992) and weighing 3.72 kg. the participant was physiologically stable, with his condition improving, while being cared for in the high care unit positioned in side-lying in an open bassinet. he was deemed ready to commence oral feeding by the paediatrician and the hospital-based slt. the participant was positioned cradled in his mother’s arm for all breastfeeding attempts, which is a comfortable position for most feeders (wolf & glass, 1992). communication interaction between the mother and the participant during all feeding sessions could be described as inadequate with limited eye contact. the results of the pibbs are presented in table 2. table 2: breastfeeding results according to the preterm infant breastfeeding behavior scale. on day 6, the participant displayed some rooting, but showed shallow latching onto only the nipple, and stayed latched onto the breast for less than 1 min (see table 2). single sucks and occasional short sucking bursts of two to four sucks were observed. occasional swallowing was noticed, but coughing and choking were present. the participant was predominantly in a drowsy state before and during feeding. premature fatiguing, as well as hiccupping and yawning, were observed. the breastfeeding session lasted approximately 41 min, and the infant received ebm via the ngt to complete the feed. ebm via cup feeding was attempted by hospital staff when his mother was not available to breastfeed. the infant received intervention by the slt daily. information available from the participant’s hospital records indicated that cup feeding was successful, however, the daily slt intervention records indicated otherwise. the participant was in the quiet-sleep stage at the scheduled feeding time on day 7. he presented with closed eyes and showed no body movements even with repeated attempts at waking him for the feeding. no rooting or latching could be elicited during breastfeeding, and no swallowing was noticed. ngt feeding continued supplemental to breastfeeding, and a vfss was scheduled for the following week. during the vfss on day 13, no penetration or aspiration was identified. no residue was left on the posterior pharyngeal wall, the valleculae or the pyriform sinuses. however, the qualitative analysis revealed poor bolus formation and delayed oral transit time, which may be indicative of oral stage dysphagia (arvedson & brodsky, 2002). on day 13, the participant had regained his birth weight at 3.82 kg and was mainly breastfed, although a ngt was still in situ. the participant was awake and alert before and during feeding. he showed obvious rooting, latched deeply onto the whole nipple and some of the areola, and stayed latched onto the breast for approximately 11 min. repeated long sucking bursts (>10 sucks per burst), and repeated swallowing, indicative of successful feeding, were observed. the participant was discharged home the following day. the participant and his mother continued with antiretroviral treatment. validity and reliability the outcome measures used in the study are published and deemed valid and reliable tools to use for the assessment of infant feeding. the reliability and validity of the pibbs was considered satisfactory and increased the rigour of this study (da costa et al., 2008; nyqvist et al., 1996). the vfss is referred to as the ‘gold standard’ for examining swallowing (arvedson & brodsky, 2002), which contributed to the validity of the results. the use of two blinded raters for the vfss increased the reliability of the vfss findings. ethical considerations ethical clearance was obtained from the hospital, as well as from the faculties of health sciences and humanities of the university of pretoria prior to data collection. voluntary informed consent was requested from the mother on behalf of her infant to participate in the study and for the medical file to be perused. the mother was informed that she may withdraw her infant from the study at any time without her usual care at the hospital being affected. the participant received intervention from a hospital-based speech-language therapist from the day of the first assessment. data were handled confidentially and will be stored electronically and in hard copy according to institutional guidelines for a period of 15 years. discussion a neonate with mild hie and hiv exposure, as well as feeding difficulties, was presented. weight loss was minimal, and the participant regained his birth weight at the last assessment prior to discharge. problems with rooting and latching onto the breast were identified while the participant was treated for septicaemia and after receiving therapeutic hypothermia and continuous positive airway pressure (cpap) in the nicu. the infant presented with hypotonia initially. muscle tone problems may lead to incorrect alignment, which may contribute to feeding difficulties (wolf & glass, 1992). the participant received parenteral and tube feeding initially. feeding difficulties requiring tube or intravenous feeding are signs of cerebral depression that may likely correlate with future neurological dysfunction (genna et al., 2013). successful latching-on and breastfeeding require an intact central nervous system (radzyminski, 2005). sucking and swallowing were not optimal for breastfeeding between birth and day 13. he was hospitalised for 14 days, mainly because of an inability to complete all feeds orally, and possibly because of reduced endurance (short sucking bursts and staying on the breast less than a minute). breastfeeding sessions may have been prolonged as the mother was motivated for her son to breastfeed and was able to dedicate time to breastfeeding. the required daily volume of feeds per day was ensured with ngt feeding. successful breastfeeding was not established until the second week after birth. infants with neonatal encephalopathy because of hypoxia-ischemia may show difficulty starting to breastfeed (tamminen et al., 1983). the participant was his mother’s fourth child, and her advanced age and prior child-rearing experience may have contributed to successful feeding after 13 days. evidence also exists that an infant’s ability to increase his or her intake at the breast develops over time (nyqvist, 2001; walker, 2017). although no clinical symptoms of oral stage dysphagia, associated with hiv, were observed, the participant’s hiv exposure may have contributed to problematic feeding. hiv exposure in itself is linked with subtle neurodevelopmental difficulties (le doaré, bland & newell, 2012), which may likely influence feeding in a young infant. a neonate with mild hie may present with absent rooting and sucking reflexes initially (thompson et al., 1997), which was true in this case. however, rooting and sucking remained inadequate for breastfeeding well into the first week of the participant’s life. an absent rooting reflex may not impact an infant’s feeding functionally, but short sucking bursts may be related to swallowing difficulties (wolf & glass, 1992). the participant’s state was drowsy before and during feeding. quiet-alert, drowsy or active alert may be the optimal states to feed a neonate (arvedson & brodsky, 2002). although the drowsy state is labelled as being one of the optimal states for feeding, there may only be one specific state for a particular infant that is ideal for feeding (wolf & glass, 1992). state is also related to an infant’s neurological and medical status (wolf & glass, 1992). cerebral depression may cause reduced alertness and a lower level of consciousness in infants (genna et al., 2013). in this study, it appears that state regulation may have affected the participant’s ability to feed successfully. the instrumental assessment of swallowing revealed no aspiration, penetration or residue. however, clinical signs of aspiration, such as coughing and choking, which may also be signs of stress (arvedson & brodsky, 2002), were observed on day 6, while the vfss was only conducted on day 13. the participant received intervention from the slt during this period, which may have contributed to the improvement in the coordination of the oral and pharyngeal stages of the swallow seen on the vfss. further research is required. the vfss during the second week revealed mild symptoms of oral stage dysphagia, which is similar to the findings in the clinical assessment of breastfeeding. from this case presentation, it is clear that feeding in a neonate with hie and hiv exposure, was not only affected because of the absence of rooting and sucking reflexes, but also because of the neonate’s inability to maintain an appropriate state for feeding. the participant’s state may have been influenced by the diagnosis of hie, and his medical status, namely the provision of cpap and therapeutic hypothermia in the first days of life as well as treatment of septicaemia (wolf & glass, 1992), and the fact that he was a post-term infant, or even a combination of these factors. post-term infants (>42 weeks gestation) may often be lethargic and may have difficulty sustaining sucking (walker, 2017). because of a relatively high incidence of hie in south africa, slts working in hospitals are required to provide the earliest feeding intervention to this population of infants. this case study outlines early salient features of the breastfeeding and swallowing of a neonate with hie with hiv exposure. although hie is not a new diagnosis in an slts case load, this case study provides a novel description of the unique feeding profile that may be found in infants with hie, namely, difficulty establishing breastfeeding initially because of poor latching, inadequate sucking and mild oral stage dysphagia. these difficulties coupled with hiv exposure and inadequate state regulation may have led to a longer hospitalisation and separation from the participant’s mother. as oral motor control, muscle tone and the infant’s state are interrelated, a deviation in any of these areas will likely influence the others and may lead to dysfunction in feeding (wolf & glass, 1992). furthermore, separation of an infant and mother leads to increased stress in both and negatively influences breastfeeding (smith, 2013). in many instances, infants with mild hie may escape the attention of the slt with a large caseload, unlike the moderate or severely involved infant with hie. the long duration of feeding difficulties after the initial recovery of mild hie in the participant was unexpected. this case study highlighted the fact that feeding difficulties and prolonged hospital stay may be the result in a case of mild hie and hiv exposure, and should, therefore, form part of the slts caseload. according to gagne-loranger, sheppard, ali, saint-martin and wintermark (2016), newborns with mild neonatal encephalopathy may require more thorough monitoring as their outcomes have been less well studied. the fact that the present study describes only one case, limits the generalisation of the findings to other infants with hie. the study presented in-depth data on one case, which may point to the need to investigate the population of infants with hie as a group. a larger scale investigation of the feeding characteristics of infants with hie may therefore be valuable in order to assess the impact of hie on infants’ initial feeding attempts, feeding practices used during therapeutic hypothermia, as well as neonatal communication development of this population. the influence of hiv exposure on neonates’ early feeding skills also requires further research, as this case study cannot provide clarity on the influence of the participant’s hiv exposure on the resultant feeding outcomes of the participant. because of the design of this study, it is also not clear what the influence of intervention by the hospital-based slt was on the participant’s outcome. future research may be directed towards the feeding intervention and neuro-developmental supportive care infants with hie receive in neonatal care. the findings of this study may lead to slts’ improved understanding of the complexity of the feeding of even the mildly affected infants with hie who also have hiv exposure. conclusion the specific breastfeeding difficulties, oral stage dysphagia and inadequate state regulation in this case study of a neonate with mild hie and hiv exposure were presented in order to contribute to data on feeding characteristics of infants with hie. even though being diagnosed as having mild hie, the participant’s problematic feeding led to prolonged hospitalisation. neonates with hie and hiv exposure may have unique feeding profiles. the early involvement of the slt in the treatment of feeding difficulties in infants with mild hie was highlighted. further research investigating the feeding and communication development of infants with hie is required. acknowledgements the authors would like to express their gratitude to prof. wilma de witt and the department of paediatrics, university of pretoria, as well as the staff at the department of speech therapy and audiology, steve biko academic hospital. competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions e.k. was the principal investigator, collected and analysed the data, and wrote the article. a.k. and l.p. assisted in design of the project and contributed to the article. references arvedson, j.c., & brodsky, l. (2002). pediatric swallowing and feeding. assessment and management. (2nd edn.). albany, ny: thomson delmar learning. ballard, j.l., khoury, j.c., wedig, k., wang, l., eilers-welsman, b.l., & lipp, r. (1991). new ballard score, expanded to include extremely premature infants. journal of pediatrics, 119(3), 417–423. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0022-3476(05)82056-6 bruckmann, e.k., & velaphi, s. (2015). intrapartum asphyxia and hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy in a public hospital: incidence and predictors of poor outcome. south african medical journal, 105(4), 298–303. https://doi.org/10.7196/samj.9140 da costa, s.p., van den engel-hoek, l., & bos, a.f. (2008). state-of-the-art. sucking and swallowing in infants and diagnostic tools. journal of perinatology, 28, 247–257. https://doi.org/10.1038/sj.jp.7211924 dematteo, c., matovic, d., & hjartarson, a. (2005). comparison of clinical and videofluoroscopic evaluation of children with feeding and swallowing difficulties. developmental medicine and child neurology, 47, 149–157. https://doi.org/10.1017/s0012162205000289 dodrill, p., & gosa, m.m. (2015). pediatric dysphagia: physiology, assessment and management. annals of nutrition & metabolism, 66(5), 24–31. https://doi.org/10.1159/000381372 gagne-loranger, m., sheppard, m., ali, n., saint-martin, c., & wintermark, p. (2016). newborns referred for therapeutic hypothermia: association between initial degree of encephalopathy and severity of brain injury (what about the newborns with mild encephalopathy on admission?). american journal of perinatology, 33, 195–202. https://doi.org/10.1055/s-0035-1563712 garg, b.p. (2004). dysphagia in children: an overview. seminars in pediatric neurology, 10(4), 252–254. https://doi.org/10.1016/s1071-9091(03)00075-5 genna, c.w., levan fram, j., & sandora, l. (2013). neurological issues and breastfeeding. in c.w. genna (ed.), supporting sucking skills in breastfeeding infants (pp. 305–358, 2nd edn.). burlington, ma: jones & bartlett learning. groenendaal, f., & de vries, l.s. (2015). hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. in r.j. martin, a.a. fanaroff, & m.c. walsh (eds.), fanaroff and martin’s neonatal-perinatal medicine. diseases of the fetus and infant (pp. 904–926, 10th edn.). cleveland, oh: saunders elsevier. horn, a.r. (2013). early prediction of hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy in newborn infants in a resource-limited setting. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of cape town, south africa. kramer, m.s., & kakuma, r. (2001). the optimal duration of exclusive breastfeeding: a systematic review. geneva: world health organization. document who/nhd/01.08-who/fch/cah/01.23,2001. le doaré, k., bland, r., & newell, m.l. (2012). neurodevelopment in children born to hiv-infected mothers by infection and treatment status. pediatrics, 130, e1326–e1344. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.2012-0405 leedy, p.d., & ormrod, j.e. (2014). practical research. planning and design. (10th edn.). london: pearson. martinez-biarge, m., diez-sebastian, j., wusthoff, c.j., mercuri, e. & cowan, f.m. (2013). antepartum and intrapartum factors preceding neonatal hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. pediatrics, 132, e952–e9509. masarei, a.g., wade, a., stat, c., mars, m., & sommerlad, b.c. (2007). a randomised control trial investigating the effect of presurgical orthopaedics on feeding in infants with cleft lip and/or palate. cleft palate-craniofacial journal, 44(2), 182–193. https://doi.org/10.1597/05-184.1 meline, t. (2010). a research primer for communication sciences and disorders. boston, ma: pearson. national department of health. (2015). national consolidated guidelines for the prevention of mother-to-child transmission of hiv and management of hiv in children, adolescents and adults. pretoria: national department of health. nyqvist, k. (2001). the development of preterm infants’ milk intake during breastfeeding. journal of neonatal nursing, 7, 48–52. nyqvist, k. (2013). breastfeeding preterm infants. in c.w. genna (ed.), supporting sucking skills in breastfeeding infants (pp. 171–196, 2nd edn.). burlington, ma: jones & bartlett learning. nyqvist, k., rubertsson, c., ewald, u., & sjöden, p. (1996). development of the preterm infant breastfeeding behavior scale (pibbs): a study of nurse-mother agreement. journal of human lactation, 12(3), 207–219. https://doi.org/10.1177/089033449601200318 pereira, v., sacher, p., ryan, m., & hayward, r. (2009). dysphagia and nutrition problems in infants with apert syndrome. cleft palate-craniofacial journal, 46(3), 285–291. https://doi.org/10.1597/08-010.1 radzyminski, s. (2005). neurobehavioral functioning and breastfeeding behavior in the newborn. journal of obstetric, gynecologic & neonatal nursing, 34(3), 335–341. https://doi.org/10.1177/0884217505276283 rosenbek, j.c., robbins, j.a., roecker, e.b., coyle, j.l., & wood, j.l. (1996). a penetration-aspiration scale. dysphagia, 11(2), 93–98. https://doi.org/10.1007/bf00417897 rybak, a. (2015). organic and nonorganic feeding disorders. annals of nutrition & metabolism, 66(5), 16–22. https://doi.org/10.1159/000381373 sarnat, h.b., & sarnat, m.s. (1976). neonatal encephalopathy following fetal distress. archives in neurology, 33, 696–705. https://doi.org/10.1001/archneur.1976.00500100030012 shalak, l.f., laptook, a.r., velaphi, s.c., & perlman, j.m. (2003). amplitude-integrated electroencephalography coupled with an early neurologic examination enhances prediction of term infants at risk for persistent encephalopathy. pediatrics, 111, 351–357. https://doi.org/10.1542/peds.111.2.351 smith, l.j. (2013). why johnny can’t suck: impact of birth practices on infant suck. in c.w. genna (ed.), supporting sucking skills in breastfeeding infants (pp. 59–82, 2nd edn.). burlington, ma: jones & bartlett learning. tagin, m.a., woolcott, c.g., vincer, m.j., whyte, r.k., & stinson, d.a. (2012). hypothermia for neonatal hypoxic ischemic encephalopathy: an updated systematic review and meta-analysis. archives of pediatric and adolescent medicine, 166(6), 558–566. https://doi.org/10.1001/archpediatrics.2011.1772 tamminen, t., verronen, p., saarikoski, s., göransson, a., & tuomiranta, h. (1983). the influence of perinatal factors on breast feeding. acta paediatrica scandinavica, 72(1), 9–12. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.1983.tb09655.x thompson, c.m., van der elst, c.w., molteno, c.d., & malan, a.f. (1997). the value of a scoring system for hypoxic ischaemic encephalopathy in predicting neurodevelopmental outcome. acta paediatrica scandinavica, 86, 757–761. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.1997.tb08581.x thyagarajan, b., tillqvist, e., baral, v., hallberg, b., vollmer, b., & blennow, m. (2015). minimal enteral nutrition during neonatal hypothermia treatment for perinatal hypoxic-ischaemic encephalopathy is safe and feasible. acta paediatrica scandinavica, 104(2), 146–151. https://doi.org/10.1111/apa.12838 walker, m. (2017). breastfeeding management for the clinician. using the evidence. (4th edn.). burlington, ma: jones & bartlett learning. wolf, l.s., & glass, r.p. (1992). feeding and swallowing disorders in infancy. assessment and management. tucson, az: therapy skill builders. victoria, c.g., bahl, r., barros, a.j.d., franca, g.v.a., horton, s., krasavec, j., et al. (2016). breastfeeding in the 21st century: epidemiology, mechanisms and lifelong effect. lancet, 387, 475–490. https://doi.org/10.1016/s0140-6736(15)01024-7 volpe, j.j. (2012). neonatal encephalopathy: an inadequate term for hypoxic-ischemic encephalopathy. annals of neurology, 72(2), 156–166. https://doi.org/10.1002/ana.23647 the communicative p e r f o r m a n c e of a severely h e a r i n g i m p a i r e d a d o l e s c e n t a n n russell, b . a . ( s p . & η . t h . ) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) psychological and guidance services, cape education department, cape town summary this study describes the communicative performance of a severely hearing-impaired adolescent. the experimenter taught the subject how to play russian backgammon. the subject conversed with, and afterwards taught his mother, speech therapist, and a peer how to play the game. each dyad played the game once. videotape recordings were made of each dyadic situation. the channels of communication, both verbal and nonverbal, used by each speaker, were determined. a relational communication coding scheme, involving the analysis of requests and subsequent responses, was applied to the data. results indicate that the hearing-impaired adolescent, though not always able to hold a dominant position in a dyadic situation, was capable of expressing the same types of control as normal adults. moreover, the types of control expressed varied as a function of each contextual setting. whenever the subject did hold a dominant position, the combined verbal plus nonverbal channel was his predominant mode of communication. these findings suggest that a sociolinguistic approach provides important information regarding a hearing-impaired adolescent's communicative performance. opsomming hierdie studie beskryf die kommunikasiegedrag van 'n erg gehoorgestremde adolessent. die navorser het die proefpersoon geieei om 'n spel te speel. die adolessent het met sy moeder, 'n spraakterapeut en 'n jeugdige van dieselfde ouderdom gesels en hulle later geleer hoe om die spel te speel. elkeen van die tweetal het die spel eenkeer gespeel. videobandopnames is gemaak van elke tweegesprek. die verbale en nie-verbale kommunikasieklee, deur elke spreker gebruik, is vasgestel. 'n kommunikasie koderingskema, wat die ontleding van versoeke en daaropvolgende response behels, is gebruik om die gegewens te analiseer. resultate het aangedui dat die gehoorgestremde adolessent alhoewel hy nie altyd 'n dominante posisie gedurende 'n tweegesprek kan handhaaf nie, nogtans instaat was om dieselfde tipe beheer as normale volwassenes, te openbaar. die tipe kontrole het verskil as 'n funksie van elke situasie. wanneer die adolessent wel 'n dominante posisie beklee het, was gesamentlike verbale en nie-verbale kommunikasiewee, sy vernaamste kommunikasiewyse. hierdie bevindinge dui daarop dat 'n sosiolinguistiese benadering belangrike inligting verskaf aangaande 'n gehoorgestremde adolessent se kommunikasiegedrag. most research dealing with the language of the hearing-impaired has focused on syntax. results across all studies indicate a retardation in linguistic performance.1' 6 ' 2 4 these findings, however, do not reflect or predict how the hearing-impaired use their language for communication. communication is . . . the transmission of a message from one person to another.21 thus, it is an interpersonal, interactive process . . . realized, not only through verbal codes, but also through a matrix of complexly integrated coding mechanisms in a communicative context.22 the natural method of communication among the congenitally hearingimpaired is by means of signs, and most hearing-impaired parents use die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 ann russell sign language with their young hearing-impaired children. 1 7 most hearing-impaired children, however, have hearing parents who, traditionally, have been discouraged from using manual communication. in an attempt to determine the relative effectiveness of oral versus manual methods of communication, most studies 7 · 1 3 have compared hearing-impaired children of hearing-impaired parents, who use a manual method of communication, with hearing-impaired children of hearing parents, who use oral language. it has been found that hearing-impaired children of hearing-impaired parents, who use a manual method have superior communication skills.21 recently, a number of investigators 8 ' 2 6 have utilized a sociolinguistic approach to describe the semantic-pragmatic component of language in the spontaneous communication of hearing-impaired preschool children. the results have revealed a clear difference between the development of a linguistic (semantic) and a communicative (pragmatic) ability: the former ability appeared to be delayed whereas the latter was age appropriate. sociolinguistic research has investigated the ways in which children talk to adults and to one another. 2 ' 1 6 very little information, however, has been published regarding the ways in which hearing-impaired adolescents communicate. if the goal of education is to prepare them to communicate effectively in society, then information about their communicative abilities is essential. adolescence, being the age between 12 and 21 years, 2 3 is a period during which the adolescent has to learn appropriate social roles. 2 5 role relationships are implicitly recognized by the way individuals interact with each other. mishler 1 9 ' 2 0 proposes that questionsustained discourse reflects the role relationships between speakers, particularly along the dominance-submission dimension. for example, in hierarchical relationships, such as a teacher-pupil relationship, it is common for the dominant person to control the dialogue by asking questions and issuing commands.5 the subordinate person does not have these privileges, but rather . . . 'is expected to respond to the imposed tasks'. 5 adolescence is a time of flux,11 and, during this period, an adolescent manifests both dominant and submissive traits in his/her struggle to establish a sense of identity. 2 3 hearing-impaired adolescents, how. :ever,-have, been ,described.as "timid.", 1! ."-passive", " s h y " 1 8 and .--. "more introvertive and submissive than those with normal h e a r i n g " . 1 2 this study represents a sociolinguistic analysis of the communicative performance of a severely hearing-impaired adolescent in three conversational settings: with his mother, his speech therapist, and a peer. in addition, each dyad will be involved in a game of russian backgammon: an interpersonal communicative task selected from the literature.1 01 5 the writer will attempt to answer the following questions: how does a severely hearing-impaired adolescent use his/her existing language to communicate? has a severely hearingthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication of a hearing-impaired adolescent. 5 impaired adolescent the requisite skills for successful interpersonal interaction? what is his/her style of interaction in a dyadic situation along a dominant-submissive dimension? can a severely hearingimpaired adolescent express control or authority in a social interaction? if so, in what specific ways is control accomplished? which channel(s) of communication will be used? m e t h o d o l o g y aim to describe the communicative performance of a severely hearingimpaired adolescent. a total description of an adolescent's communicative abilities would be so detailed as to defy analysis. for the purposes of this study, therefore, the complexity of this process was reduced by analysing it within a sociolinguistic framework, in which primary emphasis was given to the interrogative units. an interrogative unit has been defined as "three successive utterances": (1) the question, (2) the response from a second speaker, and (3) the confirmation from the initial questioner.1 9. subject (s) description the s used in this study was a white, south african, english-speaking, congenitally severely hearing-impaired male, aged 17 years. the s was fitted with 2 hearing aids and has had a consistent history of hearing aid usage, as well as speech and hearing therapy, since 18 months of age. the oral method was used extensively and intensively in the home, both parents having normal hearing. occasionally, however, natural gesture was used to facilitate interpersonal communication. at 6 years, he was enrolled as a day student at a school for the deaf. he has not failed an academic school year, and his teachers feel that his intelligence falls within the normal range. status of the s's communication behaviour at the time of testing hearing. pure tone audiometry revealed a fairly flat, severe sensori-neural hearing loss in the s's left ear. the pta was 87 db (iso 1964). there was no response in his right ear for all frequencies at the limits of the audiometer. speech audiometry revealed an aided speech reception threshold of 30 db and a speech discrimination score of 76% at 60 db. the s's speech was characterized by a number of inconsistent articulation errors, and therefore his speech was regarded as being fairly intelligible by his speech therapist at the clinic. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 ann russell his voice was fairly high-pitched and monotonous with a densasal quality. his receptive language ability was better than his expressive ability, but both areas were restricted. an analysis of his expressive language revealed an output of predominantly 3 to 4 word utterances; specific syntactic errors related to marking of tense, plurality, and possessives; inconsistently correct determiner and pronoun usage; ellipsis of large segments, inconsistent tense shifting; and poor cohesion. in terms of pragmatics, he was able to use the informing and ritualizing functions, and express his feelings, in certain situations. participant selection participants were selected in accordance with the following criteria. a. they were required to be well known to the s. b. english was required to be their home language. t c. none of the participants had played russian backgammon before. in accordance with the above criteria, the s's mother (m), speech therapist at school (t), and best friend (p), who also attended the school for the deaf, were selected. thus, one participant was selected out of each of the following primary social relationships: parent-child, teacher-pupil, and friend-friend.4 procedure (1) the experimenter (e) taught the s how to play a game known as russian backgammon. as far as possible, the ε used a verbal plus gestural and/or manipulative channel of communication to describe each item of game-information. once the s had been taught how to play the game, the ε played the game once with him to observe whether or not he had understood the rules of the g a m e . 1 4 (2) the test procedure the ε ushered each dyad, that is, the s and one participant, into a room and they were asked to discuss (i) television watching; (ii) school; (iii) extra-mural; and (iv) holiday activities as naturally as possible. it was suggested that the participant initiate the conversation by asking the s if he had watched "tv last night". each dyad was told that when they had finished discussing the 4 activities, the s was going to teach the participant how to play a game. as soon as the instructions had been given to the dyad, the/e left the room and began videotaping their , interaction through^a one-way mirror. using a sony video recorder^ testing was complete as soon as the dyad had finished playing the game. the dyadic interactions were evenly spaced over a period of 8 days and all testing took place in the afternoon when the ambient noise level was low. s each dyadic interaction was orthographically transcribed by the ε from the video recordings. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981' r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication of a hearing-impaired adolescent. 7 analysis of the data the analysis procedure was applied to the entire length of each conversation and explanation of the rules, as well as the first 5 minutes of each game. approximately 25 minutes was analysed for each dyad. the channels of communication used by the s and each participant rating of the nonverbal aspects of communication used by the s and each participant was done by the ε and a qualified speech therapist who was familiar with the informal gesture system used at the school for the deaf. the ε and the rater independently analysed each utterance. the symbols v, g/m, or vg/m were written on the transcription sheets next to each utterance to denote whether the verbal (v), gesture and/or manipulative (g/m), or verbal plus gesture and/or manipulative (vg/m) channel of communication had been used. the ε and the rater then compared their analysis. where there were discrepancies, the videotapes were replayed until exact agreement was reached. the coding and mapping procedures all questions (and commands) on the transcript sheets were located and marked " q " . any utterance following a question (or command) was marked " r " , as it might be a response to i t . 1 9 if the third speaker, in a sequence of 3 successive utterances, was the same person as the questioner, then the third utterance was counted as a confirmation and marked " c " . 1 9 nonverbal modes of communication, viz., gesture and/or manipulation of game material which served as questions, responses, or confirmations was also included. the interrogative units, consisting of successive question-responseconfirmation (q-r-c) — utterances were then "mapped" on the transcript sheets as follows: . q τ : who brings you supper? r s : we just help ourselves c τ : oh sometimes the ror c-utterance in one interrogative unit (iu) also included a question. this question then initiated a second iu which was connected to the first through the fact of their having this utterance in c o m m o n . 1 9 if the question was through the r-utterance, the connection was referred to as arching; if it was through the c-utterance, it was referred to as chaining.1 9 arching and chaining were "mapped" on the transcript sheets as follows: , q μ : did you watch tv last night? -r s : yes chaining ^ c / q μ : how much did you watch? a r c h i n g < ^ r / q s : how much? ~c/r μ : hm -c s : a little bit die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 ann russell the analysis procedure the mapped units of dialogue were then analysed. the ε worked through each successive q-r-c-utterance in 7 scans. scan 1: the q-utterances were analysed in terms of various categories, such as, request for permission, statement in request form,3 etc. each type of question was tabulated and a count made. scan 2: commands were analysed in terms of their being "single" or "multiuttered". scan 3: the r-utterances were analysed to ascertain whether the initial question or command was accepted, rejected, or evaded. scan 4: the r-utterances were analysed to ascertain whether they contained a question, thereby initiating a second iu, referred to as an arched iu series. scan 5: the c-utterances were analysed to ascertain whether they contained a question, thereby initiating a second iu, referred to as a chained iu series. scan 6: a chained iu series was further analysed to ascertain whether the r-utterance in the first iu was accepted, rejected, or evaded. scan 7: the questions and commands were analysed to ascertain whether they were dominant (d) or submissive (s) bids. a d bid was based on the rationale that the speaker assumed he had the power to make such a demand. an s bid was based on the rationale that the speaker was dependent upon the responder.3 every d and s bid, response, and iu was tabulated and a count made. the numerical scores for each interaction were then transferred from the transcripts to a score sheet. reliability of the coding, mapping, and analysis procedures was obtained for each dyad. the first 5 minutes of each game was analysed for realiability by the ε and a qualified speech therapist. interreliability agreement for these 3 procedures was 73%. the following areas were then investigated: (1) the styles of interaction: the ratio of d or s bids. (2) the manner in which control or authority were expressed. • according to bedrosian and prutting,3 a speaker can express control in any one of the following ways: (a) when the majority of his bids (either d or s) is accepted; (b) when the listener is not allowed to respond to the bids due to multi-uttered questions/commands, a statement following, the bid, the speaker answering his own question, or when n o r e s p o n s e is expected; (c) by the use of arching; and (d) by the use of chaining only when all of the bids in the iu series are accepted. ; the e, therefore, examined the scoreisheets to ascertain: (i) the types of control, and i (ii) the frequency with which they were expressed by the s and each participant. | the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication of a hearing-impaired adolescent. 9 (3) the channels of communication used by the s and each participant. only 3 different communication channels were considered: a verbal (vocalization only); a gestural (hand movements in the air) and/or manipulative (touching game materials), and a verbal plus gestural and/or manipulative. the s's and each participant's use of these channels was evaluated. the above procedures were used to analyse the speech used by the s and each participant (a) while conversing; (b) during the s's explanation of the game; and (c) whilst playing the game. results a n d discussion o f results ( 1 ) the s's style of interaction following the previously mentioned mapping and coding procedures,3 the frequencies of d and s bids, responses, and interrogative units were tallied for the s and all of the participants in each interaction (see table i for an example of how s interacted with his mother). conversational setting while conversing with his μ, t, and p, the s made 0, 8, and 1 d bid, respectively. the μ, t, and p, however, made 24, 66, and 7 d bids, respectively. therefore, it can be said that they held the dominant positions during their conversations with the s. the above results correspond with those obtained by mishler, 2 0 who found that adult-initiated conversations have a higher number of d bids than those initiated by children. explaining the game while explaining the game to his m, the s and his μ both offered 17 d bids. therefore, neither held the dominant position. during his explanation of the game to his τ and p, however, the s offered 19 and 37 d bids, respectively, while τ and ρ offered 9 and 3, respectively. thus, it can be said that the s held the dominant position during his explanation of the game. playing the game while playing the game, the s made more d bids than any of the participants. therefore, he held the dominant position each time he played the game. the overall results indicate that the s held a dominant position in 5 out of 9 settings (55,5% of the time), a submissive position in 3 out of 9 settings (33,3% of the time), and neither a dominant nor a submissive position in one setting (11,1% of the time). thus, by being able to hold both a dominant and a submissive position, the s manifested both dominant and submissive traits. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 ann russell i i i i i i i i i i i i i | m m | ' i i i i i i i i i i i i i i i ι i | tt co ι—ι rj rj νηνόοονν i i' ι ι ι ι — i i i i i i r ι i i i i i i | ι·_ u c 3 ffl q ^ i s q ^ s s " s "v, » sjis'c g-os s ο ο e (a (λ 1) ... a s s: ζ i »·§ ξ< £ ^ ό 1 8 ° : co c c _ co ο q s s d • s ^ s s i s-b χι ·κ uxj-ϊ « υ -si, ^(ϋ ^ ο υ ω j3 υ .2 α ΐλ ω co υ > y ω< the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication, of a hearing-impaired adolescent. 11 ( 2 ) expression of control s expressed all types of control while interacting with his μ, t, and p. the types of control expressed, however, varied as a function of each communicative task. for example, the s used chaining while conversing with all the participants. mishler 2 0 found that adults took control of a conversation through questions that served to chain succeeding units together. therefore, by using chaining, the s was attempting to take control of each conversation in an adult-like manner. ( 3 ) channels of communication used by the s and each participant table ii indicates that while conversing with his μ, t, and p, the s used the v channel more frequently than the g/m or vg/m channels. table ii also indicates that the s used the vg/m channel more frequently than the v or g/m channels while explaining and then playing the game. on average, the s used the vg/m channel 84% of the time. table ii also indicates that whenever the s held a dominant position, he used the vg/m channel of communication. this indicates the relative efficiency of a combined oral-manual compared to an oral — or manual — only mode of communication.2 1 table ii: predominant channels of communication used by the subject and each participant in each contextual setting channels of communication most frequently used dyad s μ s τ s ρ conversing v v* v v* v vg/m* explaining vg/m + v vg/m* vg/m vg/m* g/m playing vg/m* vg/m vg/m* vg/m vg/m* g/m * indicates the dominant speaker in each dyadic interaction + indicates that neither speaker held the dominant position in the dyadic situation. g e n e r a l discussion the results of this study seem to suggest that a sociolinguistic analysis provides important information regarding a hearing-impaired adolescent's communicative performance. for example, although the s's linguistic abilities were poorer than those of the participants', he was able to use his limited linguistic abilities to hold a dominant position in a social situation. the fact that he was able to hold both a dominant and a submissive position while interacting with the participants is significant. firstly, it indicated that he was aware of the social significance of his communicadie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 ann russell tive performance. dittmar 9 found that the average 15-year-old, with normal hearing, was just as aware of the social significance of his communicative abilities as an adult. therefore, the s's communicative performance was appropriate for his age. secondly, pervin 2 3 has stated that adolescents manifest both dominant and submissive traits in their search for a sense-of identity. these traits are reflected in the s's style of interaction. thus, the s's style of interaction also confirms that his communicative performance is age appropriate. finally, the overall results of this study reveal a distinct difference between the s's linguistic ability and his communicative performance: the former ability appeared to be restricted whereas the latter was age appropriate. implications clinical implications are: (1) the use of a sociolinguistic analysis as part of a test battery in assessing the communicative behaviour of the hearing-impaired. (2) the use of a therapeutic programme aimed at improving specific verbal, as well as non-verbal, interpersonal communication skills. implications for future research lie in: (1) the determination of the efficacy of this analysis procedure and its applicability to a diversity of language disorders and populations. these include (a) handicapped children, adolescents, and adults. this procedure has potential for use with the motorically, intellectually, as well as the audiologically impaired. (b) a large population of hearing-impaired adolescents in an attempt to assess its effectiveness with the mildly, moderately, and profoundly hearing-impaired. (2) comparing the communicative performance of (a) institutionalized and non-institutionalized hearing-impaired adolescents; and (b) hearing-impaired adolescents of hearing parents with hearingimpaired adolescents of hearing-impaired parents. references / 1. bamford, j. and mentz, l. (1979): the spoken ^ language of . hearing-impaired children: grammar. chapter 7 in speechhearing tests and the spoken language of hearing-impaired children. bench, j. and bamford, j. (eds.) academic press, inc., london. | 2. bates, e. (1976): pragmatics and sociolinguistics in child language in normal and deficient child language. morehead, d. m. and morehead, a. e. (eds.) university park press, baltimore. 3. bedrosian, j. l. and prutting, c. a. (1978): communicative the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication of a hearing-impaired adolescent. 13 performance of mentally retarded adults in four conversational settings. jnl. speech hear. res., 21, 79-95. 4. bell, r. t. (1976): sociolinguistics: goals, approaches, and problems. st. martin's press, new york. 5. blank, m. and franklin, e. (1980): dialogue with preschoolers: a cognitively-based system of assessment. applied psycholinguistics, 1, 2: 127-150. 6. brasel, κ. e. and quigley, s. p. (1977): influence of certain language and communication environments in early childhood on the development of language in deaf individuals. jnl. speech hear. res., 20, 95-107. 7. brill, r. g. (1960): a study in adjustment of three groups of deaf children. exceptional children, 26, 464-470. 8. curtiss, s., prutting, c. α., and lowell, e. l. (1979): pragmatic and semantic development in young children with impaired hearing. jnl. speech hear. res., 22, 3: 534-552. 9. dittmar, n. (1976): sociolinguistics: a critical survey of theory and application. edward arnold. 10. flavell, j. h . , botkin, p. t., fry, c. l., wright, j. w., and jarvis, p. f. (1968): the development of role-taking and communication skills in children. john wiley & sons, new york. 11. garrison, c. g. (1965): psychology of adolescence. prentice-hall, inc., englewood cliffs, new jersey. 12. goetzinger, c. p. (1978): the psychology of hearing impairment. chapter 37 in handbook of clinical audiology. 2nd edition. katz, j. (ed.) williams & wilkins co., baltimore. 13. goldin-meadow, s. and feldman, h. (1975): the creation of a communication system: a study of deaf children of hearing parents. sign language studies, 8, 224-234. 14. hoy, e. a. and mcknight, j. r. (1977): communication style and effectiveness in homogeneous and heterogeneous dyads of retarded children. american jnl. of mental deficiency, 81, 6: 587-598. 15. krauss, r. m. and glucksberg, s. (1969): the development of communication: competence as a function of age. child development, 40, 255-266. 16. kretschmer, r. r. and kretschmer, l. w. (1979): the acquisition of linguistic and communicative competence: parent-child interactions. chapter 6 in the volta review, 81, 5: 306-322. 17. meadow, k. p. (1978): the "natural history" of a research project: an illustration of methodological issues in research with deaf children. chapter 2 in deaf children: developmental perspectives. liben, l. s. (ed.) academic press, inc., new york. 18. minski, l. and shepperd, m. j. (1970): non-communicating children. butterworth and co., ltd., london. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 ann russell 19. mishler, e. g. (1975): studies in dialogue and discourse: an exponential law of successive questioning. language in society, 4, 31-51. 20. mishler, e. g. (1975): studies in dialogue and discourse: ii. types of discourse initiated by and sustained through questioning. journal of psycholinguistic research, 4, 2, 99-212. 21. moores, d. f. (1974): nonvocal systems of verbal behaviour. chapter 15 in language perspectives-acquisition, retardation, and intervention. schiefelbusch, r. l. and lloyd, l. l. (eds.) university park press, baltimore. 22. muma, j. r. (1978): language handbook: concepts, assessment, intervention. prentice-hall, inc., englewood cliffs, new jersey. 23. pervin, l. a . (1970): personality: theory, assessment, and research. john wiley and sons, inc., new york. 24. quigley, s. p., power, d. j., and steinkamp, m. w. (1977): the language structure of deaf children. the volta review, 79, 73-84. 25. schmitt, p. j. and mercaldo, d. j. (1978): training personnel for hearing-impaired adolescents. the volta review, 80, 5: 359-377. 26. skarakis, e. a . and prutting, c. a. (1977): early communication: semantic functions and communicative intentions in the communication of the preschool child with impaired hearing. american annals of the deaf, 122, 4: 382-391. 27. stuckless, e. r. (1976): manual and graphic communication. the volta review, 78, 4: 96-101. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) philips hearing aid services a d i v i s i o n of s . a . p h i l i p s ( p t y ) ltd. hearing aids amplaid audiometers group teaching systems philips hearing aid services h e a d o f f i c e 1 0 0 5 c a v e n d i s h c h a m b e r s , 1 8 3 j e p p e s t r e e t , p . o . b o x 3 0 6 9 , j o h a n n e s b u r g . philips die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28,1981 fib2399 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a filtered speech test for an aging population * hanna klein, m.a. (audiology) (witwatersrand) sub-department of communication studies, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg summary a filtered speech test was presented to sixty males and females above the age of sixty years. thirty of these subjects had complained of, and demonstrated clinically, a high frequency hearing loss for pure-tones. thirty subjects, who had never complained of a hearing loss, but who nevertheless were found to have mild-to-moderate high frequency hearing losses, were included as a control in the study. the ability to discriminate among phonetically-balanced words where certain frequencies had been filtered out deteriorated with age, although those subjects in the control group performed better than those in the experimental group. however both groups showed poorer ability to discriminate with the right ear, than with the left, or with both ears. the results seemed to indicate a particular retrocochlear involvement in an aging population. opsomming 'n gefiltreerde spraaktoets is aan sestig mans en vroue oor die ouderdom van sestig jaar voorgele. dertig van hierdie proefpersone het gekla oor 'n hoe frekwensie-gehoorverlies. hierdie toestand is ook klinies bevestig. dertig proefpersone, wat nooit oor 'n gehoorverlies gekla het nie, maar by wie geringe tot gemiddelde frekwensie-gehoorverlies nietemin gevind is, is as kontrole in die studie ingesluit. die vermoe om te diskrimineer tussen foneties-gebalanseerde woorde waar sekere frekwensies uitgefiltreer is, het met ouderdom versleg, alhoewel die proefpersone in die kontrolegroep beter as die in die eksperimentele groep gepresteer het. altwee groepe het egter 'n swakker vermoe vertoon om met die regteroor as om met die linkeroor, of met altwee ore, te diskrimineer. uit die resultate blyk dit dat daar 'n spesifieke retro-cochlieere betrokkenheid onder die bejaarde ouderdomsgroep bestaan. a common aspect noted in the texts dealing with the problems induced by the aging process, is the apparent deterioration in the communication process, and the subsequent withdrawal from social intercourse. many workers in the field have indicated that such deterioration may be related to sensory degeneration in general, and auditory disturbance in p a r t i c u l a r . 1 ' 1 ' ' 1 5 ' 2 2 a survey of the more recent literature specifically related to the auditory problems of the aged, indicates that a sensori-neural loss demonstrated by pure-tone audiometry is only one of the many audiological features that may be demonstrated clinically in the a g e d . 1 2 ' 1 6 ' 1 7 in fact, research in the areas of anatomy, physiology, and especially neurology and its allied fields, have led us to view the condition of presbycusis as an extremely complex auditory disturbance * this paper. is based on an m.a. dissertation submitted to the department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, 1976. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 88 hanna klein that may manifest itself in any, or all of the areas along the auditory pathway, from the pinna to the temporal cortex.4' 8> 1 3 > 1 8 ' 2 4 where speech signals have been distorted in terms of intensity, time and frequency, the amount of information that is conveyed by the auditory pathway to the brain is thus reduced2' 6 the normal reduction of cortical cells due to aging may limit the ability of the aged person to integrate the cues on a distorted speech test.5 matzker20 was a pioneer in the field of filtered speech tests. his study showed that normal subjects make few, if any, errors on tests of dichotic integration using a filtered speech test, whereas those with brain.stem lesions make many. in addition, he also noted how poorly the presbycusic patient performed on these tests. whereas the task of speech discrimination of phonetically-balanced words in quiet conditions is considered to give minimal information with regard to the functioning of the retrocochlear pathway, the same test used in conjunction with modifications, such as the introduction of noise, or the filtering out of crucial speech frequencies, may serve to provide more detailed information about the ability of the patient to discriminate and integrate speech c u e s . 2 ' 1 7 the introduction of a complex listening task with the aged requires careful and controlled presentation and analysis. while neurological deficits may account for a reduction in performance on complex speech tests, one has to bear in mind such psychological factors as fatigue, motivation, memory span, etc., which may mitigate against a favourable performance on such tasks.17' 21> 2 5 palva and jokinen 1 7 ' 2 1 have devised a test to determine the effects of aging on the discrimination of filtered speech. the present study was modelled on their experiments. the aim of this study was therefore to investigate certain aspects of the function of the retrocochlear pathway of the aged person, using a test of filtered speech. method subjects thirty subjects (ss) were selected on the basis of the following criteria: 1. sixty years of age, or older. 2. male and female (no predetermined sex ratio was stipulated in advance, but was dependent on the chronological sequence of referral of suitable subjects to the speech and hearing clinic, university of the witwatersrand, within the experimental period). 3. no previous or ongoing severe general illnesses. 4. negative history regarding the taking of ototoxic drugs. / 5. gradual and progressive hearing loss of recent onset. 6. negative history of noise exposure, as a result of an occupation or hobby. 7. negative history of early otological problems. table i details the number and age of subjects in the experimental group (eg). a control group was included in this study to investigate whether or not there was any difference between a group who had been referred, or self-referred, journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) filtered speech for the aged 89 male female mean age total no. 60-69 years 6 6 65.5 12 70-79 years 5 9 74.2 14 80-89 years 1 3 82.5 4 total • 12 18 74.0 30 table i. number and age of subjects in the experimental group. for an audiological assessment, and those people in the same age range who had never sought professional help, in their ability to discriminate filtered speech sounds. thirty ss were selected to match those in the experimental group for all the variables mentioned above, with the exception of a hearing loss of gradual or recent onset. table ii depicts the number and age of ss in the control group (cg). male female mean age total no. 60-69 years 6 6 63.8 12 70-79 years 5 973.8 14 80-89 years 1 3 81.5 4 total 12 . 18 73.0 30 table ii. number and age of subjects in the control group. the pilot study in view of the findings in the literature that normal ss are able to complete a test of distorted speech with few or no errors,21 it seemed essential to ascertain how people of differing ages performed on the test of filtered speech. the results thus obtained would serve to verify the accuracy of the results obtained on the test of filtered speech with the aged ss in this study, or they would demonstrate that the errors obtained were merely artefacts. thirty ss, ranging in age from 15-58 years, both male and female, were tested, in order to determine the validity of the filtered speech test. as far as possible unsophisticated listeners were selected for this aspect of the study. men and women who had not received a higher education were favoured over those who had; and only those who had no history of ear disease, medical problems or hearing loss were selected. table iii below indicates the number of ss included in the pilot study group (pg) for the filtered speech test. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) filtered speech for the aged male female mean age total no. 15-24 years 3 3 20.8 6 25-29 years 3 3 27.3 6 30-39 years 3 3 32.7 6 40-49 years 3 3 43.3 6 50-59 years 3 3 55.2 6 total 15 15 31.7 30 table 111. number of subjects in the pilot study group. preparation of material for the filtered speech test in order to standardise the presentation of the filtered speech test to all the ss, seventy-five words were recorded onto a master tape recorder nagra iii, using a tape-scotch-avi77 tenzar at 7!/2ips. a beyer mi60 200 ohm microphone was used. the following procedure was employed: preceding the recording of the words to be filtered, a 1 000 hz tone was recorded onto track one of the tape for thirty seconds for calibration purposes. the same was repeated for track two. the vu meter was set at zero decibels for calibration purposes. twenty five words each was recorded by the experimenter (e) onto channel one and two of the tape, and a further twenty-five words were recorded onto both channels simultaneously. the words were taken from the cid w-22 lists 2 and 3. no carrier phrase was used, so that minimum cues were given to the subjects. two madsen electronic speech filters were placed in the same sound-proof booth, i ac series 1604a-act, as a maico ma24 audiometer. the filter feeding the left channel of the audiometer was set up to filter out all the frequencies in the speech spectrum except those of bands 480-640 hz and 1 920-2 560 hz. using the same recording material described above, the following tape was constructed: the first word was directed to the right ear through teletronic tdh 39 earphones at bands 480-640 hz and 1 920-2 560 hz. the second word was directed to the left ear through the earphone at bands 480-640 hz and 1 920-2 560 hz. the third word was directed to both ears simultaneously, the right ear hearing the word through the earphones at 480-640 hz, and the left ear hearing the same word through the earphone at 1 920-2 560 hz. each band is too narrow for adequate discrimination. this is obviated by presentation of both frequency bands to each ear in the monaural test, and presentation of one band to each ear in the binaural t e s t . / ' 2 0 ' 2 1 a 5 second interval was left between each word recorded, and the condition was repeated twenty five times using different words. in order to verify that the filter had indeed filtered out, and maintained the desired frequencies in the speech spectrum, the resident sound engineer of the clinic analysed the frequency of certain words chosen at random; using the kay-sona-graph 6061-b. spectographs indicated that the words were not in fact being filtered as desired, and so the filters were adjusted, and the tapes remade until the spectographic analysis indicated that the desired frequency cut-outs had been obtained.14 journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) hanna klein 91 presentation of material for the filtered speech test the ss were seated in a sound-proof double suite booth, (i.a.c. 1600 act series) using the same earphones as previously described. before placing the earphones on the s's head, the ε gave the following instruction for this particular test: you are now going to hear some single words through these earphones. the words do not sound like english words because i have treated them in a special way, so that they are now distorted. you will probably not understand the words you are going to hear, but i want you to guess what they are. please do not keep quiet, rather take a guess as to what the word is. you will hear the word first in the right ear, then a different word in the left ear, and then another word in both ears. this will be repeated. the tape recording of these words will last about ten minutes. the instructions were repeated until the ε was sure the s understood, and six trial words were presented by the ε seated at the maico ma24 audiometer in the second partition of the suite. all test words were then presented at 40db above srt for each ear. all errors were phonetically transcribed for each condition, and the percentage of errors was computed. in order to determine the degree of peripheral hearing loss in the aged ss, and that hearing for puretones was normal in the pilot study group, an audiogram for each s, and their speech reception threshold, was obtained in the usual manner, prior to the administration of the test for filtered speech. results and discussion there were no significant differences between the sexes for the three test conditions for the right ear, left ear and the binaural presentation, for any of the three groups. however significant differences existed between the pg, cg, and eg in relation to scores obtained for the filtered speech test for the three test conditions. the following tables demonstrate these differences among the means. from the inspection of table iv it may be seen that the ss in the pg. scored significantly better for the three test conditions than either the eg. or cg. the significance value for the difference between the means for the three groups was p=0.05. ear pilot study group experimental group control group filtered speech right ear (fsr) 86.4 38.0 50.5 filtered speech left ear (fsl) 86.6 42.5 52.1 filtered speech binaural (fsb) 85.7 38.5 53.9 table iv. mean percentage scores for the filtered speech test for right and left, and both ears, for pg., eg. and cg. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 92 hanna klein table v demonstrates the mean scores for filtered speech for the three conditions for the age groups ranging from 15-89 years. age groups ear 15-24 25-29 30-39 40-49 50-59 aged group 60-69 70-79 80-89 right ear 92.0' 89.4 81.5 83.17 75.5 eg cg 44.1 59.4 36.6 47.3 33.5 44.4 left ear 92.0 89.1 82.0 86.17 77.6 eg cg 47.1 61.2 39.6 42.9 33.5 41.0 both ears 91.5 89.9 83.0 82.17 78.3 eg cg 44.1 63.2 33.2 43.3 34.7 45.7 table v. mean percentage scores for filtered speech for the right and left ears, and the binaural condition for the pg, cg and eg. inspection of table v once more indicates that all ss in the pg. scored significantly better than the cg. who in turn scored significantly better than the eg. for all three conditions. however it may be observed that there is a deterioration in the scores for all three conditions as the age increases; and this is so even for the pg. the results obtained in this study, which demonstrate the poorer performance with an increase in age, correspond very closely to the results obtained by palva and jokinen.21 it is interesting that none of the ss in the pg scored 100% for the filtered speech test, under any condition. these results accord closely with the studies cited by palva and jokinen.17' 2 1 ' from the results presented in table v, and those presented by palva and jokinen, l 7 ' 2 1 ' i t may be concluded that a 'normal population group' will score up to the 75th percentile for the filtered speech test, and an aged group score considerably below this level. it may be noted that in the latter two age groups studied, the difference between the means for the filtered speech conditions are less than those in the younger age groups. figure 1 details the relationship between age and scores obtained by the pg., and cg. for the three test conditions. although there are no significant differences among the three test conditions for filtered speech, it may be seen that the right ear for all three groups is slightly inferior to the left ear, and both ears. jokinen and palva17 found that their subjects, who ranged in age from 60-89 years, showed poorer discrimination in the right ear for the filtered speech test, and this score was worse than that in the left ear, and both ears, although not significantly so. figure 2 details the relationship between age and scores obtained by the pg. and eg., for, the three test conditions. it may be seen that although similar trends exist for the three test conditions in the eg., the scores are significantly poorer than those for pg. and cg. it would seem that the hearing defect which is present in this group is so severe journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 19 76 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) filtered speech for the aged 93 both ears left ear right ear 40.00 60.00 age figure 1: mean percentage scores for filtered speech for the pilot group (age 15-59) and control group (age 60-85). § s left ear right ear figure 2: mean percentage scores for filtered speech for the pilot group (age 15-59) and experimental group (age 60-85). tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak-en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 94 hanna klein that it affects all aspects of hearing ability in a more debilitating manner, than the 'normal' loss of hearing which occurs in the cg. filtered speech tests have been used to diagnose the presence of lesions in the higher auditory pathway. 2 ' 5 ' 2 0 a lesion in the auditory cortex should produce poor speech discrimination scores in the contralateral ear. where both ears respond in the same manner, and the binaural test gives similar results in both ears, one may conclude that the lesion is not at a cortical level, and in fact is probably at the brain-stem level.21 as pointed out before, the poorer responses obtained for the ss in the eg. and cg. for the right ear although not significant at the 95% confidence level, nevertheless concurred with the study by palva and j o k i n e n . 1 7 ' 2 1 they felt that the poorer scores in the right ear, giving an asymmetrical pattern to the response set, indicate that the contralateral hemisphere was not functioning as well as it did for younger people. they did not detail the exact level of the auditory pathway implicated, but because of the asymmetrical response, assumed that it must be higher than brain-stem. the better responses for the binaural condition may be due to the fact that binaural stimulation activates more neurons than monaural stimulation, and this allows greater facility for interpretation of the incoming signal. all ss in the eg. and cg. were right handed, and this would indicate a left-hemisphere dominance. irrespective of the semantic connotations of the stimulus word, it has been found that in right-handed people, the best score for complex speech tests is found at the right ear.9' 2 6 it is therefore interesting to speculate as to why the aged demonstrated poorer responses for what may be assumed to be their dominant ear. if, during aging, there is a loss of neurons, and the transmission ability in the auditory nerve is reduced because of wear and tear, it may be that the dominant route to the contra-lateral hemisphere is no longer able to transmit complex signals as effectively as the ipsilateral ear. although the dominant hemisphere has been shown to be responsible for the encoding of language and its related s y s t e m s 2 ' 1 9 ' 2 6 it may be that in the aged system the stimuli from the left ear are able to travel more easily to the left hemisphere via the right hemisphere and its connections to the left hemisphere, than through the contralateral pathway which may be reduced in efficiency. the intrinsic neural noise may be greater in the contra-lateral than ipsilateral pathway, and this may also contribute to reduced transmission in the auditory system.3 a bigger sample size may in fact have caused the differences between the right and left ear in the older groups to become significantvin addition, a detailed phonemic analysis of the type of errors made in this test may lead to greater insight as to which area is being tested since it seems that certain phonemes are mediated by either the right and/or the left hemisphere.7'27 conclusion it would seem that while pure-tone audiometry may indicate the integrity of the peripheral auditory pathway, complex speech tests may give more detailed information in regard to the function of the retrocochlear pathway. many workers have been concerned with the function of the cortex in interpreting journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) filtered speech for the aged 95 auditory signals in the aged. the conclusion which has emerged from the majority of studies has been that speech tasks under varying conditions of complexity are not correctly interpreted by the aged, as they are by younger ss who display no evidence of hearing loss. the inclusion in this study of a pilot group who ranged in age from 15 to 58 years for the filtered speech test, showed that indeed, for these tasks, younger people perform in a superior manner to people above the age of 60 who do not necessarily demonstrate a severe hearing loss. the rationale for the poorer performance of the aged ss has been attributed to a reduced number of neurons in the central nervous system, lengthening of the synapse time along the higher auditory neural pathways, and the interference of intrinsic neural noise with signal interpretation. 3 ' 1 0 ' 2 3 ' 2 8 distortions due to peripheral auditory difficulties are thus being transmitted by inefficient neural fibres in the retrocochlear pathways to a cortex where there are a reduced number of neurons. the rate of conduction is reduced and the signal may decay before it reaches the original target area. the scores obtained by the experimental group in the filtered speech test were significantly poorer than those obtained by the control group. it would seem that whatever process is responsible for the interpretation of filtered speech is more affected in the former group. since an ear asymmetry has been noted in this study, it has been suggested that the filtered speech test is sufficiently sensitive to test that area of the auditory pathway where decussation has occurred, since cortical dominance seems to be implicated where ear preference is noted.1 7' 2 1 the enigma is that there is a left-ear, rather than a right ear dominance, although all ss in the study were right-handed. it has been tentatively postulated that the dominant pathway suffers from extreme wear and tear with age, and in later years the ipsilateral pathway may invoke more efficient neural transmission. acknowledgement the writer would like to express her sincere gratitude to professor m.l. aron, head, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, for assistance with this project. this study was supported by the ernest oppenheimer post-graduate fellowship, awarded by the university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. references 1. agate, j. (1970): the practice of geriatrics. 2nd ed., heinemann medical books, london. 2. berlin, c.i. & lowe, s.s. (1972): temporal and dichotic factors in central auditory testing. in handbook of clinical audiology. (ed.) katz, j., williams and wilkins co. baltimore. 3. berruecos, p. (1970): binaural temporal integration in presbycusis. int. audio., 19,309. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 96 hanna klein 4. bocca, e. (1958): clinical aspects of cortical deafness. laryngoscope, 12, 301. 5. bocca, e. (1967): distorted speech tests. in sensori-neuralhearing processes 'anddisorders. (ed). graham, b. little, brown and co., boston. 6. carhart, r. (1973): updating special hearing tests in otological diagnosis. archives otolaryngology, 97, 88. 7. cole, r.a., & scott, β. (1974): towards a theory of speech perception. psychol. rev., 81, 348. 8. covell, w.p. (1952): histologic changes in the aging cochlea. j. gerontology, 7, 173. 9. curry, f.k.w. (1975): a comparison of left-handed and right-handed subjects on verbal and non-verbal dichotic listening tasks. cortex, 3, 345. 10. de quiros, (1964): accelerated speech audiometry. translation of the beltone institute for hearing research, 17, 5. 11. donahue, w. (1968): psychologic aspects. in the care of the geriatric patient. (ed.) cowdry, e.v. 3rd edit. the c.v. mosby co. st. louis. 12. fisch, l. (1973): special senses: the aging aduitory system. in testbook on geriatric medicine and gerontology. (ed.) brocklehurst, j.c. churchill, livingstone, edinburgh. 13. gacek, r.r., & schucknecht, i. (1969): pathology of presbycusis. int. audiol, 8, 199. 14. grieve, r.l. (1975): personal communication. 15. helander, j. (1968): some problems in the determination of the degrees of senility. in geriatric audiology (ed.) liden, g. almqvist and wiksell, stockholm. 16. jerger, j.f. (1960): audiological manifestations of lesions in the auditory nervous system. the laryngoscope, 70, 417. 17. jokinen, k., & palva, a. (1970): presbycusis. v. filtered speech test. acta oto laryngologica, 70, 232. 18. kirikae, i. (1969): auditory function in advanced age with reference , to histological changes in the central auditory system. int. audiol., 8, 221. 19. luria, a.r. (1966): higher cortical functions in man. tavistock publications. london. 20. matzker, j. (1959): two new methods for the assessment of central auditory functions in cases of brain disease. ann. otol, rhinol, laryngol., 63,1185. 21. palva, α., & jokinen, k. (1975): the role of the binaural test-in filtered speech audiometry. acta otolaryngologica, 79, 310. 22. pfalz, r., & treeck, h. (1973): why does inner-ear deafness usually commence with low intensity tones? j. audiological technique, 12, 2. 23. schonfield, d., trueman, v. & kline, d. (1972): recognition tests of dichotic listening and the age variable. j. gerontology, 27, 487. 24. schucknecht, h.f. (1967): the effect .of aging on the cochlea. in sensori-neural hearing processes and disorders. henry for. hospital international symposium. (ed.) graham, a.b., little, brown and co. boston. journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) filtered speech for the aged 97 25. smith, r.a., & prather, w.f. (1971): phoneme discrimination in older persons under varying signal-to-noise conditions./. speech hear. res., 14,630. 26. studdert-kennedy, m., shankweiler, d. (1970): hemispheric specialization for speech perception. j. acoustical society of america, 48, 579. 27. studdert-kennedy, m., shankweiler, d. (1972): auditory and phonetic processes in speech perception: evidence from a dichotic study. cognitive psychology, 3,455. 28. willeford, j.a. (1971): the geriatric patient. in audiological assessment. (ed.) rose, d.e. prentice-hall, inc. new jersey. * portable audiometers * clinical audiometers * bekesy audiometers * impedance audiometers * era/ecog audiometers * train-ears * speech training equipment * rf phonic ear systems * hearing aids the leaders for sales and service needler westdene organisation (pty.) ltd. p.o. box 28975 sandringham 2131 phone: 45-7262 45-6113/4 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 7 3 2 m e d i c a l c i t y εi o f f cor. jeppe s t r e e t q l | 1 1 i i 1 i i i i i j o h a n n e s b u r g , transvaal r e p u b l i c | ' h e a r i n g a i d c o n s u l t a n t s t p t y ] l t d . i11')))))) j/ii s a x o n w o l o 2 1 3 2 travelling audiometer noise generator. a p r e c i s i o n i n s t r u m e n t m a d e by phonak germany. a) as noise generator with the detachable loudspeaker: here is an ideal noise generator for quick check of patients, especially for babies and children. either with sinus tone or warble (pulsing sinus tone). frequency: 10002000 4000. test intensity from 35 to 90 db. b) travelling audiometer: with the headphones. this is an excellent quick-check screening audiometer with the frequencies 500 1000 2000 4000 6000. the noise absorbing headset allow testing even in noisy surroundings without a sound proof booth. other models available with different and more frequencies. please write to, or telephone oiir mr. siebold, for more details or demonstration. journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pg2-5.html editorial contextually relevant resources in speech-language therapy and audiology in south africa – are there any?* abstract in this editorial introduction we aim to explore the notion of contextually relevant resources. we argue that it is the responsibility of speech-language therapists (slts) and audiologists (as) working in south africa to develop contextually relevant resources, and not to rely on the countries or cultures where the professions originated to do so. language is often cited as the main barrier to contextually relevant resources: most slts and as are aware of the need for more resources in the local languages. however, the issue is not as straightforward as translating resources from english into other languages. the challenges related to culture, e.g. formal education and familiarity with the test situation, have to be considered, as well as the population on which norms were obtained and the nature of vocabulary or picture items. this paper introduces four original research papers that follow in this edition of the journal, and showcases them as examples of innovative development in our field. at the same time we call for the further development of assessment materials, intervention resources, and contributions to the evidence base in our context. we emphasise the importance of local knowledge to drive the development of these resources in innovative and perhaps unexpected ways, and suggest that all clinicians have an important role to play in this process. keywords: resources, development, speech-language therapy, audiology, culture ‘the responsibility to provide culturally appropriate material for our work lies within the countries to which the profession has extended.’ (watson, 2006, p. 154). around the world, the professions of speech-language therapy (slt) and audiology (a) face challenges that have been well documented: services for people with communication difficulties often have low priority in health care systems; the professions straddle education and health and are not always fully understood by each sector; resources are limited; the professions are relatively small and relatively new (enderby & emerson, 1995; hartley, 1998; nippold, 2010; swanepoel, 2006). in south africa these challenges are especially intense: there are an insufficient number of slts and as to provide services to all people; the qualified slts and as do not represent the linguistic and cultural diversity of the country’s population and are unequally distributed between the private and public sectors; the burden of infectious disease is high; health priorities often centre on saving lives rather than improving quality of life; and the research/evidence base is lacking for the context (penn, 2007). along with these challenges come opportunities. in this journal and others, much has been made of the need for the professions of slt and a to transform and develop their practice, and to make our research and practice relevant for the local context (kathard, naude, pillay & ross, 2007). one way in which the professions can start to meet these challenges is through the development of contextually relevant resources tailor-made for the local context. in this paper we explore what is meant by contextually relevant resources and practices, why developing and disseminating such resources is important, and what has already been done towards this goal, and finally we suggest ways to participate in this process. this editorial introduction grew from discussions about the papers that were submitted and accepted for publication in sajcd this year. in a seeming coincidence, each of these has innovation and development of resources and practices for the south african context as a linking theme. rogers, de wet, gina, louw, makhoba and tacon (this issue) describe the translation of the vertigo symptom scale into afrikaans, and its ability to differentiate between patients with and without vertigo. strasheim, louw and kritzinger (this issue) describe the development of a locally relevant neonatal communication intervention tool for use by clinicians in the neonatal nurseries of public hospitals in south africa. uys and van dijk (this issue) developed a music perception test for adult hearing aid users. finally, crewe-brown, stipinovich and zsilavecz (this issue) detail communication difficulties in individuals who have experienced mild traumatic brain injury, from a spouse’s perspective. this last study focused on communication in everyday contexts and explored ways in which communication can be evaluated in the absence of formal assessment procedures and functional rating scales. rather than being a coincidence, this group of papers may serve to highlight the need for development of locally relevant resources and the way in which local researchers are rising to this challenge. before describing what is meant by contextually relevant resources, it may be helpful to provide an overview of our current context. the current context: speech language therapy and audiology in south africa south africa has experienced major socio-political changes over the past 20 years. in 1994, the first democratic government was elected into power, and transformation of all sectors – health, welfare, education – began. the impact of apartheid in south africa prior to 1994 cannot be underestimated, and many of today’s pressing social issues (e.g. poverty, illiteracy) are linked to its legacy. for example, black and coloured south africans experienced great educational disadvantages that continue to have major consequences today: an estimated 15 million people cannot read or write, and one in every five south africans over the age of 20 years has not received formal education. high rates of migration, overcrowded living conditions, family violence, teenage pregnancy and substance abuse contribute to family and social difficulties in many communities (kagee, 2008). in line with its progressive constitution, south africa aims to provide all of its citizens with equal access to quality health care and education. but transformation is an ongoing process, not without challenges: despite a high incidence of hiv/aids and tuberculosis (tb), the south african public health system is characterised by sub-optimal provider-to-client ratios and insufficient material resources. education has been described as being in crisis, with national benchmarking studies suggesting that reading and writing is not being effectively taught in our schools (mullis, martin, kennedy & foy, 2007). the professions of slt and a have also changed considerably since 1994 in response to the changing milieu. swanepoel (2006) describes attempts to ‘improve imbalanced service delivery, redress teaching programmes and focus … research endeavours on the specific needs of the contexts’ (p. 264). moodley, louw and hugo (2000) describe the failure of traditional institution-based models of service delivery to reach the majority of people, especially those disenfranchised and disadvantaged, who may need our services the most. services are now focused on the community and delivered within a primary health care framework, in an attempt to address the needs of the population. in addition, the role of the slt and a in schools is being redefined. a special edition of this journal is due towards the end of the year, and will have education as its focus. south africa has a diverse multicultural and multilingual population. of an estimated 47 million people, 79% are black, 9% are coloured, 9% are white, and 2.5% are of indian/asian origin. officially there are 11 languages, but many more unofficial languages and dialects are spoken. the most widely spoken languages in the country are isizulu (23.8%), isixhosa (17.6%) and afrikaans (13.3%) (statistics south africa, 2005). however, the majority of slts and as working in the country are white englishor afrikaans-speaking; as a result, speakers of the indigenous languages have invariably been under-served (penn, frankel, watermeyer, & muller, 2009). the majority of health interactions are mediated by a third party, and more than 80% of these interactions between clients, a third party and health professionals take place across linguistic and cultural barriers (penn et al., 2009). in a small-scale survey of slts working in the western cape, pascoe, maphalala, ebrahim, hime, mdladla, mohamed & skinner (2010) found that a considerable proportion of slts are able to offer therapy in only english or afrikaans – even when working with children for whom these are second or third languages. there is a fundamental challenge here: ethical guidelines suggest that an individual should not be denied intervention because of a language mismatch with the clinician, but slts or as may not be competent to offer intervention in all languages. a study by jordaan and yelland (2003) attempted to determine how south african slts provide language intervention for multilingual language-impaired children. the results indicated that the majority of slts were providing language therapy to multilingual children in the child’s second language only – usually english. the authors attributed this to parental insistence and a lack of another common language between the slt and child. what is meant by contextually relevant resources (and why are they important)? contextually relevant resources are any tools (assessments, intervention programmes, guidelines and norms) that are available for slts and as to use with a specific population in a specific setting, and that have been developed with that population and setting in mind. many of the assessments and therapy resources in use in south africa today have been developed by clinicians and researchers in countries such as australia, the uk or the usa, and are used here in the absence of contextually relevant resources, sometimes with adaptations that make them more appropriate. in the case of standardised assessments, these will be accompanied by a set of norms against which clinicians can compare the performance of the specific individual they have assessed on a given day. this assessment procedure and comparison against norms requires a number of assumptions on the part of the clinician-assessor: firstly that the test was administered in the exact way described in the manual, and secondly that the individual-client whose performance is compared with the norms comes from the same population as that from which the norms were obtained. the first point is more easily addressed in our context, but to address the second may not be possible, and therefore results must be treated with caution. stanczak, stanczak and awadalla (2001) found that typical sudanese adults attained scores on the arabic version of the expanded trail making test that were similar to those attained by us adults with brain damage. this suggests that simply translating the language of a test does not make it appropriate for another population group, as the culture and context of the target population needs to be considered to avoid misinterpretation of results. in another study, boivin (1991) found that children in zaire performed significantly below the norms of age-matched american children on a number of non-verbal assessments widely held to be ‘culture-fair’ measures of cognitive abilities. he suggested that even supposedly ‘culture-fair’ assessments have to originate from somewhere, in this case that of western psychological research and theory, which has several fundamental underpinnings and assumptions about the way the world works that may be inappropriate when used elsewhere. in south africa, wilson and moodley (2000) determined that the use of the cid w22 wordlist (a speech discrimination test developed in the usa and widely used by south african audiologists) was problematic because normal-hearing, first-language south african english speakers performed more poorly than their us counterparts on whom the norms are based. pahl and kara (1992) assessed 60 typically developing children in south africa using the renfrew word finding scale, a test which has been developed and standardised in the uk. even though the south african children were first-language speakers of english with no language difficulties, a significant proportion of the children’s test scores fell in the range suggesting language difficulties. it is widely acknowledged that assessment is the cornerstone on which intervention should be built. if assessment is inappropriate or inaccurate and does not take cultural variation and the potential for cultural bias into account, assessment results will not be accurate and intervention may be inappropriate at best or harmful at worst (carter, lees, murira, gona, neville & newton, 2004). since it is the ethical and professional responsibility of slts and as to provide an equitable and quality service to all, the importance of using culturally fair assessment tools cannot be overemphasised. similarly, using inappropriate assessment tools in research can confound results and lead to biased conclusions. irrespective of the languages involved, it is clear that translation of a test does not necessarily make it suitable for use in another setting with a different culture. not only assessment but intervention too should be appropriate for the culture. vocabulary, stereotypical concepts, high-frequency words, body language and gestures differ between cultures and languages. it may be necessary to look at the language structure of words in different languages, because some intervention strategies commonly used with one language may not be applicable when used with another. for example, cuing words using the initial consonant sound as for english (greenwood, grassly, hickin & best, 2010) may not work well with languages such as isixhosa or sesotho, which typically begin with a vowel sound (gxilishe, 2004). we know that intervention is more valid when it is relevant and culturally acceptable, and therefore it must be tailored specifically to the culture of the individual and the community culture (hartley, murira, mwangoma, carter & newton, 2009). there is growing recognition of the necessity for developing or adapting assessment tools and procedures to match the needs of the populations. carter et al. (2004) emphasise the need to develop culturally appropriate materials that meet the needs of a specific culture, and to take cultural variation and potential cultural bias into consideration. in their kenyan-based study they found that the following factors should be taken into account by clinicians assessing or treating children from a culture different to their own: the influence of culture on performance, familiarity with the testing situation, the effect of formal education, and picture recognition. gladstone, lancaster, umar, nyirenda, kayira, van den broek & smyth (2009) described a qualitative methodology using focus groups to identify contextually important concepts and developmental milestones when creating a developmental assessment tool for malawian children, rather than simply translating and adapting available ‘western’ tools. the results from their focus groups identified social milestones and social intelligence as important aspects of development for the community, which would have been neglected in a ‘western’ test. local knowledge local knowledge refers to the ‘unique locally-available knowledge, innovations, technologies, practices, resources and their utilisation for improved livelihoods, beliefs and their contribution to the wellbeing of communities’ (nhemachena, chakwizira, dube, maponya, rashopola & mayindi, 2011, p. 2). authors such as pillay (2003), kathard et al. (2007) and joubert (2010) have variously described the origins and flawed epistemologies of the slt, a and occupational therapy (ot) professions in south africa. essentially, in these (and other) professions, ways of working have been developed in the western world that may not be appropriate for other cultures and contexts. joubert (2010), writing about ot, a profession that shares much in common with ours, describes ‘a coming of age … a stage now when [we] have used [our] resilience to really change those flaws of the past … now recreating a new and more robust and appropriate africanised epistemology’ (p. 26). kathard (2005) describes a troubled and contradictory professional identity, but suggests that the way we view ourselves as professions is not set in stone and is in the process of transforming. we believe that the innovative development work described in the journal signals a coming of age in our professions, although clearly there is much more to be done. local knowledge must be valued and used to inform the development of contextually relevant resources. to illustrate this, we use the case of isixhosa phonology. while there is a substantial amount of research surrounding children’s speech sound acquisition in english, most of this has been conducted with children in europe, north america and australia. to date, there is no tool available to comprehensively assess isixhosa phonology. there are standardised assessments of children’s speech that have been developed in other parts of the world, e.g. the goldman-fristoe test of articulation (goldman & fristoe, 1986). using the picture stimuli from this test with isixhosa-speaking children would be helpful in providing some insight into the child’s difficulties and can be used to provide some qualitative information. in the survey by pascoe et al. (2010), it was found that western cape slts rely largely on informal assessments when evaluating children’s speech. they make adaptations to formal assessments, as well as using other informal assessments of their own design. more than 50% of therapists indicated that they make adaptations to formal tests to better suit the population, e.g. translating the assessment and using more contextually relevant pictures. these slts will often omit items or sections of formal assessments that are not appropriate for their clients, and will administer tests in non-standardised ways, e.g. repeating instructions or test items. however, the isixhosa phonemic inventory contains consonants that do not occur in english and may not be elicited by these pictures. the clinician would need to know what the correct vocabulary items/names were and what the correct production of the names are. s/he would need to know what the vowel and consonant inventory of isixhosa looks like to know whether the child’s inventory was complete or not. once this information was gathered s/he would need to know whether the child’s speech was acceptable/typical for the child’s age: what are the typical processes used in isixhosa and when do they appear/disappear? these processes might not be the same as for english, given that the language structures are different, e.g. isixhosa does not typically have closed syllables and therefore final consonant deletion would not be expected. this illustration shows not only the need for development of resources, but also the need for development of local knowledge to drive the process. it illustrates that starting from a blank page may in fact be easier than trying to adapt something that has been developed for an entirely different population, in a different place, speaking a different language. gxilishe (2004) conducted a study in the western cape, looking at the acquisition of clicks by isixhosa-speaking children. he found that at the onset of speech (approximately 1 year of age) isixhosa-speaking children begin using three basic clicks. such studies are important in advancing our knowledge of speech and language development in the local context; however, further research is needed. contextually relevant resources – what has been done (and where is it hiding)? this edition of sajcd showcases some original research around the development of locally relevant resources. the two audiology papers (rogers et al., this issue; uys et al., this issue) add to a small but growing body of research in the field of south african audiology. panday, kathard, pillay and govender (2007, 2009) as part of an ongoing larger project have described the development of isizulu speech materials for use in speech audiometry, and khoza, ramma, mophosho, and moroka (2008) have examined alternative ways of carrying out speech audiometry with bilingual tswana/english speakers. while much of the research in audiology focuses on development of assessment materials, there is also work that has focused on development and evaluation of interventions (e.g. pienaar, stearn & swanepoel, 2010) and culturally relevant local knowledge regarding hearing impairment (de andrade & ross, 2005). in this issue, the paper by strasheim and colleagues focuses on the development of an early intervention tool applicable to the local context. both slts and as participated in the first phase of the study, which aimed to identify specific needs regarding clinical resources for use in neonatal nurseries. participants noted that culturally appropriate instruments were needed specifically for parent guidance and staff/team training. in response, the next phase of the study focused on development of a neonatal communication intervention programme for parents, the aim of which was to inform parents about prematurity and ways of developing early communication development. handouts were written in english and isizulu. the final phase of the study saw the piloting of the programme with two therapists. in education, wium, louw and eloff (2010) developed a continuing professional development (cpd) programme for educators to support them in their teaching of literacy and numeracy. other studies that have focused on development of slt assessments include fouche and van der merwe (1999), who described the development of a sepedi speech intelligibility test, and buitendag, uys and louw (1998), who evaluated the suitability of the afrikaanse reseptiewe woordeskattoets (afrikaans receptive vocabulary test). watt, penn and jones (1996) examined the ecological validity of a test battery for evaluating communicative effects of closed head injury. the study (this issue) by crewe-brown and co-authors details the communicative difficulties faced in their daily life by individuals with mild traumatic brain injuries (mtbis). using a case study approach, the authors show the value that ‘significant others’ can bring to understanding and supporting the individual with mtbi, an approach that could have far-ranging applicability in our context. in this section we have highlighted some of the work that has been done in our fields, rather than carrying out an exhaustive review. our survey focused on studies that have been published, but there is a wealth of unpublished work that has been carried out by undergraduate students for their final-year projects or by postgraduate students. the old adage ‘publish or perish’ may be particularly pertinent here, not only to individual academic careers but also to the professions as a whole: we have to share what we have done in order to advance our knowledge. numerous authors have urged us to carry out more research and publish our findings: swanepoel (2006) calls for more studies in the local context to determine the prevalence of hearing loss and accurately describe the status of services currently available for those with hearing impairments in south africa. without this information, swanepoel argues, legislative support and associated funding will not be forthcoming. penn (2007) decries the lack of quality, local research and urges all slts and as – and especially clinicians – to carry out research. while our agenda may be to develop local knowledge and resources, we should not be limited to publishing our work in local journals only, since many of the issues relevant to our context will have relevance for other developing settings, and there is worldwide interest in the unique languages and mix of cultures of our country. re-inventing the wheel or borrowing from our friends? indigenous knowledge can be effectively combined with external or scientific knowledge during the innovation process. hartley et al. (2009) reviewed the literature related to service development for individuals with communication difficulties in developing contexts. they suggest that western techniques and interventions cannot be rolled out to african countries without appropriate adaptation because of cultural and language differences; environments and climates and stages of social development. however, they also noted that ‘with cooperation, flexibility and humility, nations could work together to their mutual advantage using the underlying principles learnt in the west, together with local knowledge to develop appropriate training and services (p. 279)’. while it may be necessary to start from a blank page, free of assumptions, there is of course much valuable information and many lessons to be learnt from resources and knowledge developed elsewhere. publishing or presenting work at conferences is a valuable way to share resources and knowledge through a common forum which could advance the development of such assessments in a systematic manner and ensure that new slts and as or those setting up services would not have to ‘re-invent the wheel.’ joubert (2010) acknowledges the importance of developments around the world and suggests that it would be ‘both naïve and foolish not to prepare south african occupational therapists to be able to work anywhere in the world. it is however of foremost importance that they are competent in dealing with the particular health needs of south africa’s diverse population where the need for appropriate health care is greatest’ (p. 22). we believe the same is true for south african slts and as, whether working in education or health. priorities and next steps in south africa there is a great need to develop contextually relevant resources for our professions. clinicians in the study by pascoe et al. (2010) noted that an assessment tool in the most dominant languages in the western cape would be of value to them, and they suggested that this would increase their level of confidence when working with multilingual children. there is also a need for intervention resources and studies on the south african population in order to build an evidence base for the different approaches used. it may be that the natural order of this development is for assessment materials to be most needed and developed first, followed by the intervention tools and then the systematic evaluations thereof. but clinicians should be driven by their own needs. what is it that is needed to maximise our role? how can we add more value and relevance to the work we do with individuals with communication and swallowing difficulties? we should not only look to others to meet these needs, but should use our own knowledge, that of our colleagues and that of the clients we serve to move forward in this task. there is nothing wrong in starting small: modifying wordlists, devising new protocols and reflecting on our daily practice – all are valuable beginning points. we must collaborate with each other at a national level and share the gains we make. a national project under the leadership of associate professor shajila singh of the university of cape town focuses on the development of materials in the indigenous local languages and aims to encourage such collaborations and develop knowledge of the process. this is not a project for academics only, but also for all slts and as, as well as colleagues working in disciplines such as linguistics and psychology. in essence, our answer to the question posed in the title of this paper is: yes – there are contextually relevant resources for slts and as. however, as for mahlalela-thusi and heugh (2010), who examined the development of textbooks in the indigenous languages in southern africa, there is a great need for development of more resources, and further work to improve quality of the resources available for local populations. further, these resources need to be published and shared so that we can build on what has been done. this paper has aimed to move away from mere rhetoric and focus on the practicalities of our challenge. our hope is that all slts and as – especially clinicians who bemoan the lack of suitable resources – will be inspired to start innovating, collaborating and sharing. the papers that follow showcase some of the varied ways in which this can be done. acknowledgements. the authors gratefully acknowledge the support of lucretia petersen, and helpful comments from two anonymous peer reviewers. michelle pascoe vivienne norman department of health and rehabilitation sciences university of cape town *this title is adapted from the title of a paper by mahlalela-thusi and heugh (2010) entitled ‘terminology and school books in southern african languages: aren’t there any?’ references boivin, m.j. (1991). the effect of culture on a visual–spatial memory task. journal of general psychology, 118, 327-334. buitendag, m., uys, i., & louw, b. (1998). afrikaanse reseptiewe woordeskattoets (arw) : suitability for a group of non-standard afrikaans speaking children. south african journal of communication disorders, 45, 11-29. carter, j., lees, j., murira, g., gona, j., neville, b., & newton, c. (2004). issues in the development of cross-cultural assessments of speech and language for children. international journal of language and communication disorders, 40(4), 385-401. de andrade, v., & ross, e. (2005). beliefs and practices of black south african traditional healers regarding hearing impairment. international journal of audiology, 44(9), 489-499. enderby, p., & emerson, j. (1995). does speech and language therapy work? a review of the literature commissioned by the department of health . london: whurr publishers. fouche, s., & van der merwe, a. (1999). sepedi test for speech intelligibility. south african journal of communication disorders, 46, 25-35. gladstone, m., lancaster, g., umar, e., nyirenda, m., kayira, e., van den broek, n., & smyth, r.l. (2009). perspectives of normal child development in rural malawi – a qualitative analysis to create a more culturally appropriate developmental assessment tool. child care health and development, 36(3), 346-353. goldman, r., & fristoe, m. (1986). goldman-fristoe test of articulation. ags: circle pines, mn. greenwood, a.l., grassly, j., hickin, j., best, w. (2010). phonological and orthographic cueing therapy: a case of generalised improvement. aphasiology, 24(9), 991-1016. gxilishe, s. (2004). the acquisition of clicks by xhosa speaking children. per linguam, 2, 1-12. hartley, s. (1998). service development to meet the needs of ‘people with communication disabilities’ in developing countries. disability and rehabilitation, 20(8), 277-284. hartley, s., murira, g., mwangoma, m., carter, j., & newton, c.r.j.c. (2009). using community/researcher partnerships to develop a culturally relevant intervention for children with communication disabilities in kenya. disability and rehabilitation, 31(6), 490-499. jordaan, h., & yelland, a. (2003). intervention with multilingual language impaired children by south african slts. journal of multilingual communication disorders, 1, 13-33. joubert, r. (2010). exploring the history of occupational therapy’s development in south africa to reveal the flaws in our knowledge base. south african journal of occupational therapy, 40(3), 2126. kagee, a. (2008). adherence to antiretroviral therapy in the context of the national roll-out in south africa: defining a research agenda for psychology. south african journal of psychology, 38, 413-428. kathard, h. (2005). clinical education in transition: creating viable futures. international journal of speech language pathology, 7(3), 149-152. kathard, h., naude, e., pillay, m., & ross, e. (2007). improving the relevance of speech-language pathology and audiology research and practice. south african journal of communication disorders, 54, 5-19. khoza, k., ramma, l, mophosho, m., & moroka, d (2008).speech reception threshold testing in second language english speakers in south africa. south african journal of communication disorders, 55, 26-39. mahlalela-thusi, b., & heugh, k. (2010) terminology and school books in southern african languages: aren’t there any? in b. brock-utne, z. desai, m.a.s. qorro,& a. pitman (eds). language of instruction in tanzania and south africa: highlights from a project. (comparative and international education: a diversity of voices, v. 5; pp. 113-132). rotterdam: sense publishers. moodley, l., louw, b., & hugo, r. (2000). early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: a transdisciplinary model of service delivery, south african journal of communication disorders, 47, 25-40. mullis, i.v.s., o’martin, m., kennedy, a.m., & foy, p. 2007. iea’s progress in international reading literacy study in primary schools in 40 countries. boston college: timss & pirls international study centre. nhemachena, c, chakwizira, j, dube, s, maponya, g, rashopola, r., & mayindi, d. (2011). integrating indigenous knowledge systems (iks) in improving rural accessibility and mobility (in support of the comprehensive rural development programme in south africa). southern africa transport conference. international convention centre (icc), csir, pretoria, 11-14 july 2011. nippold, m. (2010). back to school: why the speech-language pathologist belongs in the classroom. language speech and hearing services in schools, 41, 377-378. pahl, j., & kara, m. (1992). the renfrew word finding scale: application to the south african context. south african journal of communication disorders, 39, 69-73. panday, s., kathard, h., pillay, m., & govender, c. (2007). development of a zulu speech reception threshold test for zulu first language speakers in kwa zulu-natal. south african journal of communication disorders, 54, 111-122. panday, s., kathard, h., pillay, m., & govender, c. (2009). the homogeneity of audibility and prosody of zulu words for speech reception threshold (srt) testing. south african journal of communication disorders, 56,60-75. pascoe, m., maphalala, z., ebrahim, a., hime, d., mdladla, b., mohamed, n., & skinner, m. (2010). children with speech difficulties: an exploratory survey of clinical practice in the western cape. south african journal of communication disorders, 57, 66-75. penn, c. (2007). ‘don’t give me the theory, just tell me what to do in therapy!’: the slippery slope challenge for the south african professions of speech-language pathology and audiology. south african journal of communication disorders, 54, 13-17. penn, c., frankel, t., watermeyer, j., & muller, m. (2009). informed consent and aphasia: evidence of pitfalls in the process. aphasiology, 23, 3-32. pienaar, e., stearn, n., & swanepoel, d. (2010). self-reported outcomes of aural rehabilitation for adult hearing aid users in a south african context. south african journal of communication disorders, 57, 4-14. pillay, m. (2003). cross-cultural practice: what is it really about? folia phoniatrica et logopaedica, 55, 293-299. stanczak, d.e., stanczak, e.m., & awadalla, a.w. (2001). development and initial validation of an arabic version of the expanded trail making test: implications for cross-cultural assessment. archives of clinical neuropsychology, 16, 141-149. statistics south africa (2005). census 2001: census in brief. retrieved on 10 may 2009 from http://www.statssa.gov.za/census01/ html/cinbrief/cib2001.pdf swanepoel, d. (2006). audiology in south africa. international journal of audiology, 45, 262-266. watson, r.m. (2006). being before doing: the cultural identity (essence) of occupational therapy. australian occupational therapy journal, 53: 151-158. watt, n., penn, c., & jones, d. (1996). speech-language evaluation of closed head injured subjects in south africa: cultural applicability and ecological validity of a test battery. south african journal of communication disorders, 43, 85-92. wilson, w.j., & moodley, s. (2000). use of the cid w22 as a south african english speech discrimination test. south african journal of communication disorders, 47, 57-62. wium, a.m., louw, b., & eloff, i. (2010). speech-language therapists supporting foundation phase educators with literacy and numeracy in a rural and township context. south african journal of communication disorders, 57(1), 14-22. 8 5 speech-language evaluation of closed head injured subjects in south africa: cultural applicability and ecological validity of a test battery nola watt, claire penn and dilys jones department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand johannesburg abstract this paper addresses the communicative outcome of a group of closed head injured (chi) subjects in south africa. communicative outcome is evaluated on one test battery currently used for medico-legal assessments in south africa. it was found that a number of the tests were sensitive to breakdown in this sample, but that the demographic factors of first language and pre-injury education significantly affected performance on some tests. many test performances were significantly related to return to work, thus confirming the importance of communicative skills in the workplace, and the speechlanguage pathologist's role in vocational assessment and rehabilitation. opsomming hierdie verslag spreek die gevolge van geslote hoofbeserings by 'n groep suid-afrikaanse proefpersone aan. 'n spesifieke toetsbattery wat tans in suid afrika vir regsmediese evaluasies gebruik word, het gedien as instrument om die proefpersone se kommunikasievermoens te ondersoek. sommige toetse was sensitiefvir kommunikasie uitvalle maar sekere demografiese faktore, b.v. moedertaal en opvoedkundige peil voor die hoofbesering, het die proefpersone se prestasie op sommige toetse be'invloed. baie van die toetse het 'n duidelike verband getoon met die proefpersone se vermoe om terug te keer werk toe. die belangrikheid van kommunikasie-vermoens binne die beroepskonteks en die spraak-taalterapeut se rol in beroepsevaluering in rehabilitasie word dus bevestig. key words: closed head injury; communicative outcome; ι introduction j speech-language pathologists in the medico-legal field are called upon daily to describe and quantify communicative deficits as a result of: closed head injury (chi). these clinicians, however, have little empirical or policyrelated guidance as to which tests are indeed sensitive to breakdown in the south africian population, and how performance may be affected by certain demographic and cultural factors, for example, educational level. this is important to know when one considers the vast discrepancies in such socio-economic indicators in south africa. further, the medico-legal professional is required to prognosticate, beyond a reasonable doubt, whether or not the patient will be able to return to work. again, this is currently taking place with little scientific insight regarding which communicative skills are most important in the workplace, and which tests are most useful to measure them. the above difficulties faced by the south african speechlanguage pathologist are highly significant in the light of the extremely high incidence of chi in south africa, cited as 316/100 000 (nell & brown, 1990), approximately 50% higher than abroad. in addition, dwindling insurance and return to work third party payouts demand that professionals ensure that they are using culturally appropriate and functionally valid tests in order to ensure the strongest possible case for their clients. this study was hence aimed at determining the sensitivity of a test battery already used by a practice involved in medico-legal assessments in south africa, the effects on performance of early demographic and injury variables, and which test performances were most closely related to return to work. in order to contextualise the results of this study, previous research documenting communication breakdown following head injury will be discussed, as well as the impact of early variables on test performance and research documenting the relationship between communicative skills and vocational success. 1. communicative breakdown following closed head injury there has been much debate in the literature regarding the nature and mechanisms underlying communication breakdown following chi. early studies in the 1970's and early 1980's assessed and classified the communicative breakdown using traditional aphasia tests. the incidie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 86 dence of classical aphasic syndromes found within the chi population ranges from 2% (heilman, safran & geschwind, 1971) to 30% (sarno, 1988). hartley and levin (1990) point out that the lower incidence of classical aphasias tends to be associated with samples of consecutive cases of head injury, which contain a wide range of severity levels, while higher incidences have been reported in samples of chi admissions to rehabilitation centres, which are likely to be mainly severe injuries. a number of specific linguistic deficits have also been found to occur relatively often in the absence of a classical aphasic syndrome, and have been termed "subclinical aphasia" (sarno, 1988). the most common deficit is anomia, measured usually on a confrontation naming task such as the boston naming test (heilman, safran & geschwind, 1971; levin, grossman & kelly, 1976). verbal fluency tasks have also been found to be sensitive measures of anomia (walsh, 1991). in addition, impaired auditory comprehension, particularly of complex commands as measured by the token test, has been found to occur relatively often (levin et al., 1976; sarno, 1988; kreutzer, gordon, rosenthal & marwitz, 1993). more recently, the focus of chi assessment has shifted to the investigation of more naturalistic forms of language use, these being discourse and pragmatics. it is not surprising that the use of such different tests has led to the identification of very different types of communication impairment. one of the earliest studies which investigated the discourse of chi individuals found it to be characterised by impoverished productivity in terms of the number of words produced, the rate of speech, and the percentage of syllables in mazes (wyckoff, jensen & lapointe, 1984). the amount of content was also consistently reduced in comparison to controls, and the subjects used fewer cohesive ties per communication unit. the researchers summarise these findings by describing chi discourse as limited in "quantity, efficiency and connectivity" (wyckoff, jensen & la pointe, 1984). other studies investigating discourse have confirmed deficits in the following areas: discourse cohesion (mentis & prutting, 1987; liles, coelho, duffy & zalagens, 1989);" discourse superstructure and story grammar elements (chapman, culhane, levin et al., 1992; liles et al., 1989); coherence (chapman et al., 1992; glosser & deser, 1990); and productivity (hartley & jensen, 1991; mentis & prutting, 1987; liles et al., 1989). within the realm of pragmatics, deficits have been found to occur in all three of the nonverbal, interactional and propositional aspects of communication (hartley, 1990). nonverbal deficits in the paralinguistic features of speech fluency, rate of speech and voice quality (marsh & knight, 1991), fluency (hartley & jensen, 1991) and prosody (milton, prutting & binder, 1984) have been found. the interactional aspects of communication have also been found to be significantly affected after chi. in particular, inappropriate turn-taking behaviours have been noted (milton, prutting & binder, 1984; coelho, liles & duffy, 1991), as well as deficits in "partner-directed behaviour" (marsh & knight, 1991). mcdonald (1993) demonstrated significant difficulties in the chi individual's ability to meet the informational needs of their listeners. similarly, irvine (1984) demonstrated disrupted interpersonal skills on scale a (response to interlocutor) on penn's (1985) profile of communicative appropriateness. finally, researchers have demonstrated difficulties in the propositional the sou nola watt, claire penn and dilys jones aspects of communicative interaction following chi. these include difficulties in topic selection, maintenance, relevance and quantity of verbal output (milton et al., 1984; ehrlich, 1988; penn & cleary, 1988). at this point, it seems to be important to gather information about how a south african sample of chi individuals perform on a battery of tests in order to confirm patterns of breakdown, as well as which tests appear to be most sensitive in measuring this breakdown. 2. impact of early variables on outcome/ test performance given the fact that about 88% of the south african population is not first language english speaking, and that about 25% are illiterate (central statistical services, 1994), understanding the impact of demographic factors on test performance is of paramount importance in south africa. in most outcome studies, researchers have attempted to identify factors in the head-injured patient's past and related to the injury itself that can account for the outcomes they describe. few studies, however, have investigated the impact of these effects on communicative outcome. it is only very recently that a systematic discussion of the possible effects of such factors on test performance has been made and preliminary investigations carried out (coelho, 1995). the need for this empirical information in south africa is great, particularly in the light of our multicultural and multilingual society, with such a large disparity in socio-economic status among its inhabitants. it is almost impossible to comment on test performance without it. 3. impact of communicative deficits on return to work (rtw) return to work following head injury abroad, and particularly in south africa is a major concern as insurance and other resource allocations for chi individuals diminish. however, few outcome studies have included the assessment of the impact of communication difficulties on the chi patient's ability to return to work. in the studies which have assessed this relationship, researchers have used widely differing measures of communicative functioning, from standardised aphasia tests to questionnaire items, thus making comparison and generalisation difficult. also, researchers have tested their subjects at different times-post-onset.those testing in the acute stage have measured the prognostic value of early language breakdown on later vocational performance (i.e., communicative functioning as a predictor of future performance), while those who have tested breakdown in the chronic stage measure the relationship between communicative and vocational performance at the same time (i.e., communicative functioning as an indicator of outcome). this is useful in that it allows researchers to make hypotheses as to which deficits are affecting performance at the time of testing. testing in the acute stage of recovery, dresser, meirowsky, weiss et al.(1973) found that the early presence of aphasia was a significant predictor of future unemployment. similarly, najenson, groswa'sser, mendelsohn and hackett (1980) found the early presence of aphasia to have an important influence on rehabilitation outcome, and a significant effect on vocational functioning on follow-up. / african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech-language evaluation of closed head injured subjects in south africa: cultural applicability and ecological validity of a test battery 87 considering communication breakdown as a late indicator of outcome, brooks, mckinlay, symington et al. (1987) found that ratings of communicative impairment by relatives or significant others were significantly related to failure to return to work. two particular communication deficits, the ability to carry on a conversation and to understand a conversation, were highly significant. in a metaanalysis of the predictors and indicators of work status, crepeau and scherzer (1993) were able to compare five studies which investigated the relationship between communicative abilities and return to work, and found their results to be relatively homogeneous with communicative abilities having a moderate correlation with return to work. however, they stress that due to the differing measures used in the various studies, it was difficult to come to a closer understanding of exactly what fosters a strong relationship between communication and return to work. it appears that no detailed study correlating performance on a battery of communication tests and vocational status has been carried out to date. also, it is only recently that speech-language pathologists have begun working in close collaboration with the vocational rehabilitation team in attempting to evaluate systematically the communicative requirements of the workplace (fraser & baarslag-benson, 1994). the aim of this study, therefore, was to evaluate a particular communication test battery, developed and currently used by a speech therapy practice in south africa, in terms of the following three aspects: 1. sensitivity to breakdown in the chronic stage of recovery; 2. effects of early demographic and injury variables on test performance; and, 3. the relationship between test performance and occupational status. methodology a group of fifty chi subjects who had been evaluated for medico-legal purposes, was selected based on the following criteria: | 1. the subjects had to speak! english as a first langauge, or at least with enough proficiency not to require an interpreter during testing. this limited the cultural diversity of the sample, but aimed to ensure greater test validity; i 2. the subjects had to be of working age at the time of testing; j 3. sufficient information had to be available regarding all the communicative functioning, demographic and injury-related variables, and occupational functions for data analysis. the medico-legal reports of the subjects who fulfilled these criteria were reviewed in detail, and the relevant data was captured on a data recording form, and transferred into a computerised database devised by the first author (watt, 1996). the aim of this form was to provide a standard method of analysing the reports and recording the data. it includes 197 variables, including demographic information of age at onset, time since onset, gender, preand postinjury educational levels, preand postinjury occupational types and status; injury-related information of severity, cause of injury and surgical procedures; and early postinjury variables, e.g., therapy received. the form was designed to be similar to that designed for the model systems database for traumatic brain injury that has been established in the united states of america (dahmer, schilling, hamilton et al., 1993) in order to make it comparable to international databases and yet feasible in a country with fewer resources for detailed assessment and data gathering. table 1 summarises the demographic, injury and early post-injury factors investigated in this study. the communication parameters included on the form are comprehensive in scope and reflect the assessment battery (detailed in table 2) of the speech-language pathologists who conducted the medico-legal assessments. this battery was devised on the basis of substantial experience with a wide range of medico-legal assessments (>200), and taps a range of cognitive and linguistic domains, including hearing, motor speech abilities, receptive language, expressive language, reading, writing, verbal reasoning, and pragmatics. the battery utilises a mixtable 1: characteristics of the sample characteristic percentage age at onset mean: 29y lomo sd: 12y lomo time since onset mean: 3y 5mo sd: l y 5mo gender male 74 female 26 first language english 62 afrikaans 16 zulu/xhosa 8 sotho 6 other european 8 pre-injury matriculation 50 education certificate or less . post secondary 50 school training or qualifications pre-injury manual worker 12. occupational clerical/technical 22 type professional/executive 40 type student 26 pre-injury competitively employed 74 occupational full time status full-time student 26 injury severity mild 18 moderate 12 severe 70 therapy received speech-language 24 post-injury occupational 20 physio52 cognitive 6 counselling 2 vocational 2 social services 2 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 88 nola watt, claire penn and dilys jones ture of standardised as well as modified, adapted, and novel or clinician-constructed items that have been developed over time in an attempt to meet the unique needs of the south african head injured population. this appears to have been essential in the face of a shortage of relevant materials, and too great a dependence on inappropriate overseas tools. the form does not record exact test scores, as these are table 2: outcome on the communicative test battery task missing adequate not adequate mild moderate severe hearing 39 11 2 3 6 motor speech dysarthria ope dysphagia voice fluency 34 28 40 34 28 16 22 10 16 22 6 14 5 6 9 4 7 4 6 13 6 1 1 4 reception token test celf(ambiguities) . celf(relationships) tolc (ambiguities) normal conversation rhcb (alternate word meaning) rhcb (verbal humour) 5 9 12 26 2 28 43 18 19 15 14 42 12 2 27 22 23 10 6 10 5 11 12 12 3 6 8 2 10 8 7 6 2 2 6 2 4 1 1 expression word-finding1 verbal fluency narrative discourse procedural discourse syntax1 paraphasias1 perseverations1 serial speech 2 3 3 2 1 1 1 1 14 12 25 40 44 42 45 48 34 35 22 8 5 7 4 1 13 17 9 5 5 5 1 13 12 10 2 2 3 8 6 3 1 1 reading2 comprehension drawing inferences memory 2 11 16 31 12 17 17 27 17 7 10 7 9 13 7 1 4 3 writing3 letter formation spelling 2 4 20 28 28 18 26 11 1 4 1 ! 3 ! verbal4 reasoning speed arithmetic 4 3 27 22 19 25 18 13 1 10 1 2 ! pragmatics5 topic control relevance turn-taking quantity eye contact gesture posture 1 2 1 1 1 28 31 37 29 38 42 45 22 18 13 19 11 7 4 6 8 5 5 3 12 9 7 13 11 4 4 4 1 1 1 key: 1: assessed in spontaneous speech 2: assessed using the tolc inferences subtest and/or an informal fable task 3: assessed in spontaneous writing task 4: assessed in clinician-constructed verbal arithmetic task 5: assessed in spontaneous speech the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech-language evaluation of closed head injured subjects in south africa: cultural applicability and ecological validity of a test battery 89 often not reported in medico-legal reports, which usually describe the performance qualitatively and interpret the test scores. hence the researchers classified performance on a five point scale according to whether it was above average (1), average (2), intact but slow or showing mild difficulties (3), moderate difficulties (4), and marked or severe difficulties (5), on the basis of the medico-legal report. this appeared to be the most appropriate way of capturing the data. a measure of inter-rater reliability was calculated to ensure that the data-capturer was able to extract this information reliably. an independent speech pathologist completed the "communication" section of the database form for one fifth of the sample, and inter-rater reliability was calculated using cohens's kappa, a stringent measure of agreement that corrects for chance agreement (howell, 1992). the following mean kappa values were obtained: per subject 0.62; per construct: motor speech 0.86, receptive language 0.62, expressive language 0.81, reading 0.87, writing 1, verbal reasoning 0.6, pragmatics 0.73. these values indicate "substantial" to "almost perfect" levels of agreement (landis & koch, 1977) and suggest that the data was recorded sufficiently reliably from the medico-legal reports. the data was analysed according to the three aims. frequency counts were used to organise the group data, while relationships between variables were calculated using cross-tabulations and chi squared analysis. when sample sizes were small, fisher's exact probability test was used (siegel, 1956). the five point rating scale describing communicative performance was collapsed into two categories, namely adequate (ratings of 1 and 2) and inadequate (ratings of 3, 4 and 5) in order to be compatible with 2 x 2 contingency tables. this system is similar to the cut-off systems employed by kreutzer, gordon, rosenthal and marwitz (1993). » results and discussion / 1. sensitivity of the test battery table 2 describes the communicative performance of the subjects on the communication test battery. the column labelled "missing" indicates the number of subjects for whom data on a particular test was missing. this figure reveals how the test batteries used changed according to the needs of the patient. hearing the occurrence of a hearing loss as a result of the head injury in 22 % of this sample is substantially higher than that reported by giles and clark-wilson (1993) of 6 to 8 % of all hospital trauma admissions. it is significant that despite this high incidence, few, if any, had been referred to an audiologist before the time of the medico-legal assessment, which took place a mean of three years postinjury. reception it would appear from table 2 that the receptive battery used is indeed sensitive to breakdown in comprehension in this sample as over half of the subjects manifested difficulties on the revised token test, the subtest used from the clinical evaluation of language function (celfxprocessing relationships and ambiguities) and the right hemisphere communication battery (rhcb) verbal humour subtest. just under half manifested difficulties on the test of language comprehension (tolc) inferences subtest and the alternate word meanings subtest of the rhcb. the most discriminative test on which the most subjects performed poorly was the revised token test, which is considered to be fairly specific and highly diagnostic of comprehension difficulties (howieson & lezak, 1992). this finding confirms previous reports of the prevalence of a specific linguistic deficit in auditory comprehension in the absence of traditional aphasia following closed head injury. it is interesting to note that few of the subjects appeared impaired in spontaneous conversation. this illustrates the ease with which comprehension difficulties may be masked in conversational exchanges, very often the only medium on which other professionals base their referral to a speech-language therapist. expression the profile of communicative performance on the expressive tests clearly indicates which tests are sensitive to breakdown in this south african chi sample. nearly 70 % exhibited word finding difficulties in connected speech, which confirms previous repots (thomsen, 1975; levin, grossman & kelly, 1976; sarno, 1984), although these authors relied more on confrontational naming tasks for diagnosis. poor verbal fluency measures also confirm the previously cited sensitivity of this procedure (walsh, 1991). the narrative discourse task was the third measure which appeared to be very sensitive to breakdown. although discourse analysis was not detailed, this finding confirms others' results (kaplan, 1987; liles et al., 1989; hartley & jensen, 1991). reading the assessment of reading appeared to be well justified by the poor performance of the individuals, particularly for deeper level comprehension such as drawing inferences from the text. this is what is referred to as intratextual processing (ulatowska, chapman & johnson, 1992) such as making inferences on the tolc or recognising the moral of a fable. as with auditory comprehension, these results highlight the importance of assessing deeper comprehension abilities which would otherwise be missed in basic comprehension tasks. verbal reasoning regarding verbal reasoning abilities, the results indicate that tests of arithmetic ability, particularly those which are verbally mediated and require understanding of verbal concepts, are highly sensitive to breakdown following head injury. there was a dissociation between speed of processing and arithmetic ability on these tasks. this was characterised by very quick and impulsive calculations with incorrect answers by some and extremely slow working with ultimately correct answers by others. this has implications both for therapy and for vocational placement. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 90 nola watt, claire penn and dilys jones pragmatics the pragmatic categories included in the data recording form did not appear to be very sensitive to breakdown in this sample. this may have reflected insufficiently detailed pragmatic analysis. although there were subjects who manifested difficulties on all of the parameters described, none of these were very great, the most sensitive measure being that of topic control (44% had difficulties), quantity of verbal output (38%) and relevance (36%). the presence of these difficulties does confirm previous reports of pragmatic breakdown following head injury (milton, prutting & binder, 1984; penn & cleary, 1988). interesting significant relationships between the non-verbal behaviour of eye contact and the pragmatic behaviours of topic control and relevance were found. a possible explanation for this may be that eye contact is essential for gaining important cues from the listener which help to regulate and monitor turns and relevance. in summary, it appears that the communication test battery used for this sample was sensitive enough to detect breakdown in performance in nearly all communicative spheres. most of the difficulties experienced by this group confirm previous studies and indicate that such difficulties are persistent, even up to a mean of three years post-injury. these findings have great implications for the role of the speech-language pathologist in the rehabilitation of this clinical population. 2. early predictors of communicative performance the impact of early demographic and injury related variables on communicative test performance was assessed. table 3 highlights the relationships that were found to be significant at alpha=0.05. the subjects' first language was found to be the most significant factor affecting communicative performance. this indicates that first language european language speakers (i.e., english, afrikaans and other european languages e.g., german) performed significantly better than subjects whose first langauge was an african language. this does not necessarily mean that these tests have no place in a south african test battery, as a number of these tests were also significantly related to occupational outcome (see below). however, it does mean that therapists must be aware of this factor, as well as which tests are not affected by first language (e.g., pragmatic functioning) and therefore to include these for valid testing. it is striking how small an impact injury severity had table 3: significant predictors of communicative outcome (df=l except for severity where df=2) test gender first language education severity hearing 0.034 * dysarthria 4.24x10s ** ope problems 0.016 * token test 0.031 * understanding normal conversation 0.033 * ; narrative discourse 0.040 i " · i procedural discourse 0.050 * ι 1 reading: comprehension 5.47xl0"3 ** 0.015 * reading: drawing inferences x2=4.41 * . • reading: memory 0.044 writing: letter formation x2=4.11 verbal reasoning: arithmetic x2=5.09 * significant at 0.05 ** significant at 0.01 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech-language evaluation of closed head injured subjects in south africa: 9 1 cultural applicability and ecological validity of a test battery particularly interpersonal aspects of communication, on general communicative functioning in the chronic stage of recovery. severity of injury was significantly related only to motor speech abilities as manifested in dysarthria and ope difficulties. it appears that, as far as the speechlanguage pathologist is concerned, injury severity may have less of a role to play, particularly in expressive and receptive language, than other cultural factors such as first language and pre-injury education. 3. relationship between communicative performance and rtw performance on each of the communicative tests was correlated with the occupational status of the subjects, i.e., employed or unemployed. table 4 highlights the significant relationships found between test performance and return to work. these results show that adequate performance on at least one test in nearly every communicative domain was significantly related to the ability to return to work. this is encouraging regarding the communicative domains tapped in the assessment as well as the tests used for this purpose. the importance and value of the revised token itest appears to be underscored by the very strong relationship between performance on this test and return to work. since, as mentioned previously, the token test has been described as relatively specific to, and therefore strongly diagnostic of langauge comprehension difficulties (howieson & lezak, 1992), this finding can confidently be interpreted as confirming the essential role of high level language comprehension abilities within the functional context of the workplace. the strong relationship between the reading measures and return to work is possibly related to the nature of the sample, the majority of the subjects having been employed in clerical or professional capacities in which reading is essential. these results reveal the high premium placed on literacy in the vocational context. surprisingly, none of the pragmatic parameters were significantly associated with whether or not the subject was able to return to work. this is a counter-intuitive result, as many authors suspect that poor pragmatic skills, table 4: communicative tests significantly related to rtw (df=l) : construct 1 test ; x 2 value motor speech ope 6.148 * reception token test 13.500 *** expression narrative discourse 4.627 * reading comprehension 9.52 *** \ inferences ' \ 5.110 * memory 7.77\ ** verbal reasoning speed 5.101 * * significant at 0.05 ** significant at 0.01 *** significant at 0.005 would have severe vocational consequences after head injury (hartley, 1995; sarno, 1988). it was, however, found that poor turn-taking abilities and abnormal quantity of verbal output were significantly associated with the downgrading of those who had returned to work (x2 values of 5.109 and 7.670; alpha=0.05). in other words, a significant number of the working subjects who presented with these symptoms had been downgraded at work, either in terms of the time commitments of the job (e.g., part time v. full-time) or the employment conditions (e.g., competitive v. sympathetic employment). these findings add more empirical weight to the importance of communicative skills in the workplace, and thus address an important gap in the literature. they also provide convincing support for the functional appropriateness of the tests in this communicative test battery, particularly for the medico-legal assessment, as well as the role of the speech-language pathologist in the vocational rehabilitation team for chi individuals. summary and conclusions these results have important clinical implications for the south african speech pathologist involved in the assessment of chi individuals. they indicate that communicative breakdown is pervasive and persistent following closed head injury, affecting particularly high level linguistic abilities, and that there are a number of tests available, both standardised and non-standardised, which are sensitive to this breakdown. clinicians must, however, be aware of how the south african cultural and socio-economic milieu affects performance on some of these tests, and interpret their results accordingly. they should also be sure to include tests in their battery appear to be less affected by such variables (e.g., verbal reasoning tasks and pragmatic skills evaluation). in addition, these results highlight the integral importance of communication skills in the workplace, and how poor performance on a number of tests is significantly related to a failure to return to work. there is a need for the development of policy guidelines by the profession as a whole to guide the continued development and fine-tuning of a sensitive, valid and reliable test battery for the assessment of communicative breakdown following chi. it is evident that at the present time south african clinicians are using vastly different assessment tools with little empirical basis for their test selection. results such as these may have relevance in the setting up of such guidelines. such a policy may also then have implications for the clinician's dealings with insurance companies, providing therapists with greater empirical and political support for their evaluation procedures, results and prognoses. a number of implications for future research also emerge from these finding. firstly, the study should be expanded and replicated with a larger number of subjects, particularly of the smaller groups represented here, such as the first language african language speakers. in doing so, the methodology of this study has implications for the possible compilation of a national database for head injury in south africa such as that set up abroad (dahmer et al., 1993). a necessary precursor to such a database would be consensus regarding a common minimal test battery for all participating speech therapists and, again, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 92 nola watt, claire p e n n a n d dilys jones the issue of policy guidelines governing such decisions is applicable here. the empirical confirmation of the importance of communicative skills in the workplace also has implications for further research. while these results confirm the importance of specific communicative skills, such as comprehension, expression and reading, in successful vocational o u t c o m e , further r e s e a r c h is n e c e s s a r y to determine whether the pattern of demands differs across different occupations. atool for measuring the communicative demands of a specific workplace is currently being developed at the university of the witwatersrand, which will be used by clinicians to conduct on-site j o b skill assessments. in conclusion, this study has highlighted the areas of persistent w e a k n e s s in communicative functioning of a group of chi subjects in south africa. tests sensitive to this breakdown have been identified, as well as which test performances are affected by demographic factors such as pre-injury educational level and first langauge. in addition, test performances most closely related to successful reintegration in the workplace have been identified. these results support the role of the speech-language pathologist in the long-term rehabilitation of c h i individuals particularly in the occupational sphere. r e f e r e n c e s brooks, n., mckinlay, w., symington, c., beattie, a. & campsie, l. (1987). return to work within the first seven years of severe head injury. brain injury, 1, 5-19. central statistical services (1994). october household survey statistical release. p0317. chapman, s.b., culhane, k.a., levin, h.s., harward, h., mendelsohn, d., ewing-cobbs, l., fletcher, j.m. & bruce, d. (1992). narrative discourse after closed head injury in children and adolescents. brain and language, 43, 42-65. coelho, c.a. (1995). discourse production deficits following traumatic brain injury: a critical review of the recent literature. aphasiology, 5, 409-429. coelho, c.a., liles b.z. & duffy, r.j. (1991). analysis of conversational discourse in head injured adults. journal of head trauma rehabilitation, 6, 92-99. crepeau, f. & scherzer, p. (1993). predictors and indicators of work status after traumatic brain injury: a meta-analysis. neuropsychological rehabilitation, 3, 5-35. dahmer, e.r., schilling, m.a., hamilton, b.b., bontke, c.f., englander, j., kreutzer, j.s., ragnarsson, k.t. & rosenthal, m. (1993). a model systems database for traumatic brain injury. journal of head trauma rehabilitation, 8, 12-25. dresser, α., meirowsky,, α., weiss, g., mcneel, m., simon, g. & caveness, w. (1973). gainful employment following head injury. archives of neurology, 29, 111-116. ehrlich, j .s. (1988). selective characteristics of narrative discourse in head-injured and normal adults. journal of communication disorders, 21, 1-9. fraser, r.t. & baarslag-benson, r. (1994). cross-disciplinary collaboration in the removal of work barriers after traumatic brain injury. topics in language disorders, 15, 55-67. giles, g.m. & clark-wilson, j. (1993). brain injury rehabilitation. a neurofunctional approach. london: chapman and hall. glosser, g. & deser, t. (1990). patterns of discourse production among neurological patients with fluent language disorders. brain and language, 40, 67-88. hartley, l. (1995). cognitive-communicative abilities following brain injury. a functional approach. california: singular • publishing group. hartley, l.l. (1990). assessment of functional communication. in d.e. tupper and k. d. cicerone (eds). the neuropsychology of everyday life: assessment and basic competencies, (chap. 6). boston: kluwer academic publishers. hartley, l. & jensen, p. (1991). narrative and procedural discourse after closed head injury. brain injury, 5, 267-285. hartley, l. & levin, h. (1990). linguistic deficits after closed head injury: a current appraisal. aphasiology, 4, 353-370. heilman, k.m., safran, a. & geschwind, n. (1971). closed head trauma and aphasia. journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, 34, 265-269. howe, j.r. & miller, c.a (1975). midbrain deafness following head injury. neurology, 91, 50-60. howell, d.c. (1992). statistical methods for psychology. belmont california: duxbury press. ' howieson, d.b. & lezak, m.d. (1992). the neuropsychological evaluation. in s. c. yudofsky and r. e. hales (eds). the american psychiatric press textbook of neuropsychiatry. (2nd ed.) (chap. 5). washington: american psychiatric press, inc. irvine, l. (1984). the communicative and cognitive deficits following closed head injury. unpublished undergraduate research report. university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. kaplan, d. (1987). discourse production in closed head injury. unpublished master's dissertation. university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. kreutzer j.s., gordon, w.a., rosenthal, m. & marwitz, j. (1993). neuropsychological characteristics of patients with brain injury: preliminary findings from a multicentre investigation. journal of head trauma rehabilitation, 8, 47-59. landis, j.r. & koch, g.g. (1977). the measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. biometrics, 33, 159-174. levin, h.s., grossman, r.g. & kelly, pj. (1976). aphasic disorder in patients with closed head injury. journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, 50, 183-193. liles, b., coelho, c., duffy, r. & zalagens, m. (1989). effects of elicitation procedures on the narratives of normal and closed head-injured adults. journal of speech and hearing disorders 54, 356-366. marsh, n.v. & knight, r.g. (1991). behavioral assessment of social competence following severe head injury. journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 13, 729-740. mcdonald, s. (1993). pragmatic language skills after closed head injury: ability to meet the informational needs of the listener. brain and language, 44, 28-46. mentis, m. & prutting, c.a. (1987). cohesion in the discourse of normal and head-injured adults. journal of speech and hearing research, 30, 88-98. milton, s., prutting, c. & binder, g. (1984). appraisal of communicative competence in head injured adults. in r. brookshire (ed.). clinical aphasiology conference proceedings (pll4-123). minneapolis: brk publishers. najenson, t., groswasser, z., mendelsohn, l. & hackett, p. (1980). rehabilitation outcome of brain-damaged patients after severe head injury. international rehabilitation medicine, 2, 17-22 nell, v. & brown, d.s.o. (1990), epidemiology of traumatic brain injury in johannesburg: morbidity, mortality and etiology. health psychology unit technical report no. 1: unisa and hsrc. penn, c. (1985). the profile of communicative appropriateness: a clinical tool for the assessment of pragmatics. south african journal of communication disorders, 32, 18-32. penn, c. & cleary, j. (1988). compensatory strategies in the language of closed head injured patients. brain injury, 2,3-17. sarno, m.t. (1984). verbal impairment after closed head injury. report of a replication study. the journal of nervous\and mental disease, 172, 475-479. sarno, m.t. (1988). head injury: language and speech deficits. scandinavian journal of rehabilitation medicine. suddi 17 5 5 6 4 . ' ' siegel, s. (1956). nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sciences. new york: mcgraw-hill book company, inc. thomsen, i.v. (1975). evaluation and outcome of aphasia in patients with severe closed head trauma. journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, 38, 237-254. ulatowska, η. k., chapman, s.b. & johnson, j. (1992). depth of information processing for discourse in aphasic and elderly patients. unpublished research. walsh, k.w. (1991). understanding brain damage. a primer of neuropsychological evaluation. (2nd ed.) melbourne: churchill livingstone. watt, n.j. (1996) predictors and indicators of outcome following closed head injury in south africa. unpublished master's dissertation. university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. wyckoff, i.h., jensen, p.j., & lapointe, l. (1984). narrative and procedural discourse following closed head injury. itechnical paper presented at asha convention: san francisco, california. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, and critically evaluative theoretical and philosophical conceptual issues dealing with aspects of human communication and its disorders, service provision, training and policy. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications manuscript style and requirements manuscripts should be accompanied by a covering letter providing the author's address and telephone numbers. all contributions are required to follow strictly, the style specified in the publication manual of the american psychological assoc. (3rd ed., 1983) (apapub. man.), with complete internal consistency. four copies of triple-spaced high quality type-written manuscripts with numbered pages, and wide margins should be submitted. they should be accompanied by one identical disc copy of the paper. filenames should include the first author's initials and a clearly identifiable keyword or abbreviation thereof and should be typewritten on the last line of the last page of the reference list (for retrieval purposes only). as a rule, contributions should not exceed much more than 30 pages, although longer papers will be accepted if the additional length is warranted. the first page of two copies should contain the title of article, name of author(s), and institutional affiliation (or address). in accordance with the apapub. man. style (1983, p.23) authors are not required to provide qualifications. in the remaining two copies, the first page should contain only the title. the second page of all copies, should contain only an abstract (100 words), written in english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors. major headings where applicable should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements, references. all paragraphs should be indented. tables and figures which should be prepared on separate sheets (one per page), should be copied for review purposes and only the copies sent initially. figures, graphs, and line drawings that are used for publication, however, must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper, but these will not be required until after the author has been notified of the acceptance of the article. lettering appearing oh these should be uniform and professionally done, allowing for|a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should appear below it and should not be included in the illustration. the titles of tables, which appear above, the figures, which appear below, should be concise but explanatory. both should be numbered in arabic numerals in order of appearance. the number of illustrative materials allowed, will be at the discretion of the editor (usually about 6). references references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and the date, e.g., van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, after the first occurrence, et al. after the first author will suffice, except for six or more when et al. may be used from the start. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list, which should be listed in strict alphabetical order in triple spacing at the end of the article. all references should be included in the list, including secondary sources, (αρα pub. man. 1983, p. 13). only acceptable abbreviations of journals may be used, (see dsh abstracts, october; or the world list of scientific periodicals). the number of references should not exceed much more than 30, unless specifically warranted. examples locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48 339-341. penrod, j.p. (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. davis, g.a. & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca: college-hill. editing acceptable manuscripts may be returned to the author for revision. additional minor changes may also be made at this stage, but a note on the manuscript acknowledging each alteration made by the author, required. the paper is then returned to the editorial committee for final editing for style, clarity and consistency. reviewing system the peer review of refereeing system is employed as a method of quality control of this publication. peer reviewers are selected by the editor based on their expertise in the field and each article is sent to two independent reviewers to assess the quality of the manuscript's scientific and technical content. the blind peer review system is employed during which the name of the author/ authors are not disclosed to the reviewers. the editor retains the final responsibility for decisions regarding revision, acceptance or rejection of the manuscript. reprints: 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. deadline for contributions: the preferred date is the 31st may each year, but papers will be accepted until 30th june by arrangement. queries, correspondence & manuscripts: should be addressed to the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech-language-hearing association, p.o. box 600, wits, 2050, south africa. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) inligting vir bydraes die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer verslae en artikels wat gemoeid is met navorsing, of handel oor krities evaluerende, teoretiese en filosofiese konseptuele kwessies wat oor menslike kommunikasie en kommunikasieafwykings, diensverskaffing, opleiding en beleid gaan. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings sal nie artikels aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. manuskrip styl en vereistes: manuskripte behoort deur 'n dekkingsbrief vergesel te word wat die skrywer se adres en telefoonnommers bevat. daar word van alle bydraers verwag om die styl, soos gespesifiseer is in die "publication manual of the american psychological assoc. (3rd ed., 1983) (apapub. man."), nougeset te volg met volledige interne ooreenstemming. manuskripte moet getik, van hoe gehalte en in drievoud spasiering met wye kantlyne wees. vier kopiee van die manuskrip moet verskaf word. een hiervan moet 'n identiese skyfkopie van die artikel wees. leername behoort die eerste skrywer se voorletters en 'n duidelike identifiseerbare sleutelwoord of afkorting daarvan in te sluit en moet op die laaste lyn van die bladsy van die verwysingslys getik word (slegs vir naslaan doeleindes). as 'n reel moet bydraes nie 30 bladsye oorskry nie, maar langer artikels sal aanvaar word indien die addisionele lengte dit regverdig. op die eerste bladsy van twee van die afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, naam van die skrywer(s), en instansie (of adres) verskyn. in ooreenstemming met die "apapubl. man." se styl word daar n i e van skrywers verwag om enige kwalifikasies te verskaf nie. op die eerste bladsy van die twee oorblywende afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskaf word. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. hoofopskrifte moet, waar van toepassing, in die volgende volgorde verskaf word: metode, resultate, besprekings, gevolgtrekkings, erkennings en verwysings. alle paragrawe moet ingekeep word. tabelle en figure wat op afsonderlike bladsye (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie) moet verskyn, moet vir referent-doeleindes gekopieer word en slegs die kopiee moet inisieel verskaf word. figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge wat vir publikasie gebruik word, moet egter oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte wees. die oorspronklikes sal slegs verlang word nadat die artikel vir publikasies aanvaar is. letterwerk wat op bogenoemde verskyn, moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50% verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by illustrasies moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verklarings of legendes moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maar daaronder, verskyn. die opskrifte van tabelle (wat bo-aan verskyn), en die onderskrifte van figure, (wat onderaan verskyn), moet beknop, maar verklarend wees. numering moet deur middel van arabiese syfers geskied. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word. die aantal tabelle en illustrasies wat ingesluit word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). verwysings verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, b.v., van riper (1971). wanneer daar egter meer as twee skrywers is moet daar na die eerste verskaffing van al die outeurs, van et al. gebruik gemaak word. in die geval waar daar egter ses of meer outeurs ter sprake is moet et al. van die begin af gebruik word. al die name van die skiywers moet in die verwysingslys verskyn wat aan die einde van die artikel voorkom. verwysings moet alfabeties in trippel spasiering gerangskik word. al die verwysings moet in die verwysingslys verskyn, insluitende sekondere bronne, ("αρα pub. man." 1983, p. 13). slegs aanvaarbare afkortings van tydskrifte se titels mag gebruik word, (sien "dsh abstracts, october"; of the world list of scientific periodicals). die aantal verwysings moet nie meer as 30 oorskry nie, tensy dit geregverdig is. let op die volgende voorbeelde: locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341. penrod, j.p. (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. davis, ga. & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca.: collegehill. resensering resensering deur vakkundiges word toegepas as 'n metode van kwaliteitskontrole van hierdie publikasie. resenseerders word deur die redakteur geselekteer op grond van hulle spesialiskennis en elke artikel word na twee onafhanklike resenseerders gestuur om die kwaliteit van die manuskrip se wetenskaplike en tegniese inhout te beoordeel. die naam van die outeur/outeurs word nie aan die resenseerder bekend gemaak nie. die redakteur behou die verantwoordelikheid vir die finale beslissings aangaande wysigings, aanvaarding of afkeuring van die manuskrip. redigering 1 manuskripte wat aanvaar is, mag na die skrywer teruggestuur word vir hersiening. addisionele kleiner veranderinge mag ook op hierdie stadium aangebring word, maar 'n nota ter aanduiding van alle veranderinge wat op die manuskrip voorkom1, moet verskaf word. die artikel word dan aan die redaksionele kbmitee vir finale redigering van styl, duidelikheid en konsekwentheid teruggestuur. j herdrukke: 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis aan die outeurs verskaf word. sluitingsdatums vir bydraes: bydraes word verkieslik teen 31 mei elke jaar verwag, maar artikels sal nog tot 30 junie vir aanvaarding oorweeg word. navrae, korrespondensie en manuskripte: moet geadresseer word aan die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, die suid-afrikaanse spraak-taal-gehoor vereniging, posbus 600, wits 2050, suidafrika. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'ν oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings by hakkelaars maggi van wyk b.a. (log) (pretoria) skoolsielkundige dienste, posbus 1360, kempton park, 1620. opsomming 'n oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings wat in spraakopnames van 18 hakkelaars voorgekom het, is gemaak. hierdie gedragsvorm is 'n belangrike diagnostiese kriterium van hakkelspraak. buiten die voorkomsfrekwensie is veral ook die aard en plek van voorkoms van onderbrekings belangrike faktore in die diagnose van hakkel. kliniese waarnemings beklemtoon dat die woorddeelherhalings van hakkelaars van die van nie-hakkelaars onderskei kan word deur o.a. tempoverskille, onreelmatigheid, gebrek aan oorgangsformante, die gebruik van die schwa-vokaal en die voorkoms van benaderingsgedrag. ouditiewe parameters asook spektrogramme is gebruik om die hipotese te bevestig dat die tempo van woorddeelherhalings verskil van die tempo waarteen die vloteenhede van spraak geuiter word. ook is hierdie herhalings opmerklik onreelmatig en het die verlangde oorgangsformante nie by herhalingseenhede voorgekom nie. dit is egter nie die schwavokaal wat gebruik word nie en dit is onwaarskynlik dat die herhalings dien as benadering tot die verlangde oorgang. hierdie bevindings is van belang vir die differensiaaldiagnose tussen hakkelspraak en normale onvlotheid. summary an audio-visual analysis was conducted on the repetitions of parts of words which occurred in the recorded speech of 18 stutterers. this behaviour manifestation is an important diagnostic indication of stuttering. not only the frequency of interruptions, but also their nature and place of occurrence, are important factors in the diagnosis of stuttering. clinical observations stress that the stutterer's repetitions can be distinguished from those of nonstutterers with relation to differences in tempo, irregularity, lack of formant transitions, the use of the schwa vowel and the occurrence of approximation. auditory parameters as well as spectrograms were used to confirm the hypothesis that the tempo of repetition of parts of words differs from the tempo at which the fluent units of speech are uttered. these repetitions are also notably irregular and necessary formant transitions do not occur in the case of the units which were repeated. however, it was not the schwa vowel which was used, and it is uolikely that the repetitions served as an approximation of the desired transition. these findings are of importance for the differential diagnosis between stuttered speech and normal nonfluency. die bydraes ten opsigte van hakkel in die literatuur skep dje indruk van groot meningsverskille en duidelike uiteenlopendheid/hoewel daar reeds baie beskrywings van hakkel gegee is, bv. die baie omvattende beskrywing deur wingate,32 kom hierdie beskrywings oorwegend daarop neer dat hakkel beskou kan word as onderbrekings in die vlotheid van verbale uiting. die frekwensie van onderbrekings belemmer kommunikasie,5 maar dit blyk ook uit baie beskrywings dat veral die aard en plek van voorkoms van hierdie onderbrekings belangrike faktore in die diagnose van hakkel is. nie alle onderbrekings van spraakvloei kan as hakkel beskou word nie. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings 65 vlotspraak is hierargies-gefntegreerde gedrag: hierargies, omdat dit odgebou is uit verskillende elemente op die klank-, lettergreep-, woorden sinsvlakke28 en ge'fntegreerd, omdat hierdie elemente so ingeskakel is dat oorgangsvlotheid gemanifesteer word. hierdie oorgangsvlotheid word egter onderwerp aan aanvaarbare onderbrekings soos pouses, ritmiese patroonvorming, tempoveranderinge, intonasieen klempatrone en tussenwerpsels wat dui op onstabiliteit op die supramorfemiese vlakke. beskrywings gegee deur boehmler,7 webster en brutten,30 peters en giles21 en van riper28 dui daarop dat onstabiliteit wat op morfemiese vlakke manifesteer as hakkel gediagnoseer word. hoewel hakkel baie meer behels as die definisies gegee deur bg. persone, kan hierdie simptome as die kerngedrag van hakkel beskou word. webster en brutten30 skryf die meningsverskille toe aan die konglomeraat van gedragsvorme wat voorkom by hakkelaars en lewer dan ook 'n pleidooi aan ondersoekers om 'n noukeurige analise te maak van komponente gedragsvorme wanneer hakkel bestudeer word. die verklaring vir die verskynsel dat gevestigde hakkelaars nie dieselfde merkbare simptome vertoon nie,9 kan moontlik gesoek word in die primere en sekondere gedragsvorme waarin die hakkelmoment gedifferensieer kan word. hierdie stelling steun brutten en shoemaker8 se siening dat die hakkelaar se self-gedifferensieerde, "onwillekeurige" gedrag in die primere en sy "willekeurige" gedrag in die sekondere kategorie val. meeste waarnemers stem saam dat hakkelgedrag met tydsverloop verander. die gevestigde hakkelaar toon 'n groot verskeidenheid gespanne worstelings en morbiede emosionele reaksies wat min verband hou met sy vroeere hakkelgedrag. hakkelteoriee is dikwels in konflik met mekaar omdat hulle gebaseer is op die aard van die afwyking nadat dit alreeds ten voile ontwikkel het. aangesien sekondere gedrag verskillend ontwikkel by verskillende persone, sal navorsing wat gebaseer is op hierdie gedragsvorme lei tot verwarring.28 dit dien as motivering waarom 'n gedragsvorm wat min of meer universeel by hakkelaars van alle ouderdomme voorkom, bestudeer moet word. montgomery en cooke20 noem dat woorddeelherhalings een van die universeel herkenbare simptome van hakkel is. dit is een van die kerngedragsaspekte van hakkel, aangesien dit een van die primere kenmerke is wat die ontwikkelende hakkelaar onderskei van die nie-hakkelaar. daarom is woorddeelherhalings as studieterrein in hierdie navorsingsprojek geneem. herhalingsgedrag by hakkel vlotheid word versteur wanneer 'n spraakeenheid deur die een of ander onderbreking geskei of verwyder word van die res van die uiting.6 hierdie onderbrekings kan verskeie vorms aanneem soos bv. tussenwerpsels, herhalings (van klanke, lettergrepe, woorde en frases); hersienings, verlengings, gebroke woorde (pouses en eksplosiewe vrylatings) en selfs onvolledige of onvoltooide s i n n e . 7 ' , 5 ' 4 herhalings is een van die basiese onderbrekings wat voorkom in die spraak van die hakkelaar en word gekenmerk deur 'n duplikasie van sekere spraakelemente. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) maggi van wyk die_belang van herhaling as diagnostiese kriterinm herhalings is 'n belangrike diagnostiese kriterium, aangesien hakkel herken kan word aan onderbrekings op morfemiese vlak, terwypnie hakkelende spraak gekenmerk word deur onderbrekings op suoramorx v i t r if is veral op *** 1. die onderskeiding tussen aanvangshakkel en normale onvlotheid by kmders. aanvangshakkelaars toon konsekwent lettergreepen klankherhalings, asook klankverlengings, terwyl woorden fraseherhalings eerder kenmerkend is van die normale onvlotheidsperiode m<32 2. die verskil tussen hakkel en onvlotheid in nie-hakkelaars se spraak· veral boehmler7 het hier gedui op die diagnostiese belang van lettergreepen klankherhalings. die feit dat sheehan2 4 persentasiegewys bewys het dat sodanige herhalings by uitstek op die bestaan van hakkel dui terwyl woorden fraseherhalings meestal by nie-hakkelaars voorkom) beklemtoon nie alleen die diagnostiese waarde van hierdie gedragsvorm nie, maar onderskryf ook wingate33 se siening dat herhalings d™ ondaengl f ΐ ϊ y a , " ά ϊ k e m 8 e d r a g v a n h a k k e l vorm. ander outoriteite onderskryf ook hierdie bev.ndings en dui aan dat fraseherhalings, tussenwerpsels en pouses aanvaarbare onderbrekings van normale spraak die omvang en aard van herhalinpsgehrao id™ h f i s t u p e r , i n 5 h i e r v a " ί d k e e r s t e n s n o d i g o m ooreenstemmings te ldentifiseer. relatiewe waardes en verskille sal eers daarna geidentifiseer en duidelik word.3' navorsingsresultate dui reeds op sekere'ooreen emas s n e f e r e o n i a p r r i e r h a l i r ; g s b v " d p e s e n p l e k v a n v o o r k o m s · sowel as sekere onderske.dende eienskappe bv. duur.3' verder kan die aard van herhalingsgedrag ook reeds gekoppel word aan die erns van hakkel l8<23 daar bestaan egter nog 'n dringende behoefte aan die bestudering van die aard van kerngedrag as diagnostiese kriterium.28 kliniese bevindings o l t e z z t ^ v o , 8 c n d e v e r s k y n s d s ^ herhalingsgedrag fs i n t v t k i l l e ; b ° n r f s l m a t i g , h e i d : g e b r e k a a n oorgangsformante, gebruik van die schwa-vokaal; moonthke benaderingsgedrag; staking van lugvloeistaking van fonasie; skielike staking (teenoor geleidelike afname by die vvf w i η·[ t ) ; e " k ° " p ° u s e s t u s s e n herhalings. dit is veral die eerste toeligttng verdien ^ ^ ^ ^ w ° r d e " " a d e r e l s ! . m ™ m p 0 ^ v e d v u l c l i g e wo^ddeelherhalings. reel'matigheid in die tempo van uiting is kenmerkend van vlotspraak. so sal selfs 'n konstante tempo in die herhalings van nie-hakkelaars as aanvaarbare onderbrekings beskou kan word.^dit is egter wanneer die tempo van herhalings merkbaar verskil van die normale spoed van lettergreep uiting dat dit as aanduidend van hakkel beskou word.28 hoewel daar al navorsing gedoen is oor die duur van herhalings,2 4'3' is dit nog nie s t i t i n g n i f d u u r ^ ^ ° ~ m m e 8 n d e vloteenhefe™ the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings 67 2. die onreelmatigheid van woorddeelherhalings. 'n ander onderskeidende kenmerk van herhalingsgedrag by die hakkelaar is onreelmatigheid. 2 8 ' 2 9 van riper meen dat hierdie veelvuldige onreelmatige herhalings verband hou met die hakkelaar se onvermoe tot tydsberekening in sy spraakuiting. beech en fransella5 onderskryf hierdie teorie. kliniese waarneming dui daarop dat hierdie soekende gedrag be'invloed kan word deur faktore soos tydsdruk, wat hierdie onreelmatigheid tot gevolg kan he. 2 6 3. gebrek aan oorgangsformante by woorddeelherhalings. by vloeiende spraak word oorgangsvlotheid op die fonemiese vlak deur koartikulasie bewerkstellig.9 in die geval van herhalings by nie-hakkelende spraak kom die nodige oorgangsformante en lugvloei voor, terwyl beide ontbreek by herhalings van hakkelaars.28 hierdie verskynsel is deur stromsta25 deur middel van spektrogramme bewys. 'n verdere verwante verskynsel is van riper28 se vermoede dat die motoriese programmering belemmer word, omdat die hakkelaar sy artikulators tydens herhalings in 'n onnatuurlike posisie plaas. sheehan2 4 bewys dat 43,6% van die herhalings verkeerde klanke bevat. dit word verklaar op grond daarvan dat die hakkelaar dit te moeilik vind om die hele woord aan te durf en dus net woorddele probeer uitspreek wat die voorkoms van oorgangsformante onnodig maak.6 verskeie skrywers beklemtoon dat hakkel as 'n fonetiese oorgangsprobleem beskou kan word. 1 ' 3 3 die beskouing verskaf 'n verfyning in analise wat meer spesifiek die essensiele aard van die hakkelmoment spesifiseer. dit is egter nie maklik waarneembaar nie en soms kan dit slegs deur middel van spektrogramme uitgelig word.25 4 die gebruik van die schwa-vokaal by woorddeelherhalings. die feit dat kliniese bevindings daarop dui dat die schwa-vokaal gebruik word tydens herhalingsgedrag word o.a. deur sheehan,24 van riper28 en montgomery en cooke2 0 verklaar. navorsing bewys egter dat alleenlik in 25 % van gevalle die schwa gebruik word. 2 0 ' 2 4 in hierdie gevalle is die verlangde formantoorgange en koartikulasie dus afwesig. na aanleiding hiervan meen montgomery en cooke2 0 dat die voorkoms van die schwa in die inisiele herhaling van veelvuldige herhalings wel van riper28 se teorie in verband met soekende of benaderingsgedrag kan bevestig. dit is duidelik dat verdere navorsing in verband met hierdie verskynsel noodsaaklik is. 5. woorddeelherhalings as 'n vorm van benadering tot die verlangde fonetiese oorgang. by veelvuldige herhalings lyk dit, oppervlakkig beskou, asof dieselfde lettergreep oor en oor herhaal word. by nadere ondersoek blyk dit egter dat varierende artikulasieposisies voorkom, wat kan dui op soekende of benaderingsgedrag.30 montgomery en cooke20 kan benadering nie bevestig nie, omdat veelvuldige herhalings nie genoegsaam voorgekom het in hul proefpersone se spraak nie. dit is dus 'n faset wat verdere navorsing vereis. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol.25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 maggi van wyk hoewel die term "lettergreepherhalings" meestal gebruik word in die literatuur, was dit nie geskik vir gebruik in hierdie ondersoek nie lettergreepherhalings" sluit noodwendig ook een-lettergrepige woorde in en lg. herhalings kan ook as normale onvlotheid voorkom 3 1 die term woorddeelherhalings" is gekies om te verhoed dat verwarring by analise ontstaan. die term het in hierdie studie die betekenis van die herhaling van η lettergreep of kleiner eenheid van 'n woord.31 om hierdie rede word "vlotlettergreep" ook vervang met die term "vloteenheid." spraakanalise m.b.v. die spektrograaf hoewel daar nie 'n meer sensitiewe analiseerder van spraak is as die oor self nie,1 word die klankspektrograaf as hulpmiddel tot ouditiewe analise gebruik, aangesien dit spraak na sigbare patrone verander wat as verwysing dien vir korrekte en foutiewe artikulatoriese bewegings in aaneenlopende spraak.16 op spektrogramme kan daar ook afsonderlike sowel as aaneenlopende segmente waargeneem word 1 daar word dus van 'n oudiovisuele analise gebruik gemaak aangesien modaliteite elk op eie wyse betekenisvolle leidrade aangaande spraakdimensies verskaf. talle ondersoekers bevestig dat spektrografiese analise waardevolle inligting aangaande spraakkenmerke van spraakafwykings kan verskaf.20 2 5 ' 2 2 ' 2 8 f metode h i p o t e s e die hipotese word gestel dat woorddeelherhalings by hakkel verskil van die ooreenstemmende vloteenheid en van nie-hakkelspraak in verskeie opsigte nl.: 1. die tempo van die herhalings verskil van die normale spoed van uiting van die ooreenstemmende vloteenheid. 2. die hakkelaar se veelvuldige woorddeelherhalings vind plaas teen η onreelmatige tempo. 3. die verlangde oorgangsformante verskyn nie by die woorddeelherhalings nie. 4 g e b r u t ^ 3 v ° k a a l w ° r d d c u r h a k k e l a a r s b y woorddeelherhalings 5. die veelvuldige herhalings is 'n vorm van benadering tot die verlanede oorgang. b y / p r o e f p e r s o n e y agttien proefpersone is gebruik. hulle is geselekteer op grond van die teenwoordigheid van woorddeelherhalings in hul spraak. ouderdom of geslag is me in ag geneem nie. a1 die,proefpersone was reeds vooraf deur η paneel spraakterapeute as hakkelaars gediagnoseer. volwassenes is ook gebruik, aangesien beide kinders en volwassenes wat as hakkelaars gediagnoseer is, se spraak proporsioneel meer woorddeelherhalings toon.7 the south a frican journal of communication disorders. vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings 69 die voorwaarde was dus dat die proefpersone hierdie universele kerngedrag moes toon. geen ander spraakverskynsels of gedragsvorme wat by individuele proefpersone mag voorgekom het, het 'n rol gespeel nie. proefpersone: ouderdomme (jare) manlik vroulik 0 5 1 5 1 0 3 1 1 1 1 5 2 2 1 6 2 0 1 20 25 4 bo 25 3 ' 1 a p p a r a a t bandopnames is gemaak in 'n klankdigte kamer met 'n nagra 111 no. bh 67884 bandopnemer en mikrofoon. vir die ontlokking van spontane spraak, is t.a.t. en c.a.t. kaarte gebruik.15 spektrogramme is gemaak met 'n voice identification incorporated model 700. p r o s e d u r e s ses-minuut opnames van spontane spraak is van die agttien hakkelaars gemaak volgens die voorskrifte van johnson, darley en spriestersbach15 — "the job task". hierdie prosedures is ook aangepas vir kinders. die t.a.t. en c.a.t.-kaarte is gebruik vir spraakmonsters wat meer emosionele betrokkenheid van die sprekers geverg het. die maak van spektrogramme: altesaam 305 spektrogramme is van die herhalingsgedeeltes gemaak. betroubare prosedures is gebruik om die spektrogramme te maak en te analiseer. hier kan verwys word na hulp en aanwysings van kopp en green, 1 6 kopp, kopp en angelocci;17 potter, kopp en kopp2 2 se geskrifte. om te bepaal of die woorddeelherhalings reelmatig of onreelmatig plaasvind. dit het berus daarop dat die duur van die herhalingseenhede, asook die duur van die vloteenheid met behulp van 'n konstantheidsfaktor bereken is. die duurverskil is dan in vier kategoriee ingedeel. om te bepaal of die verlangde oorgangsformante by die eerste of enigste herhalingseenheid voorgekom het. in hierdie geval is gebruik gemaak van die eerste, tweede en derde formantposisies; die bandopnames (ouditiewe modaliteit) en die formantpatrone, om te bepaal of die verlangde oorgangsformant definitief, onwaarskynlik of glad nie voorgekom het nie. die formantpatrone is in ag geneem, omdat klanke wat gekombineer word mekaar be'invloed. dit word getoon in die oorgange van hierdie formantpatrone. hierdie oorgangsbewegings is belangrik by die herkenning van geproduseerde klanke en dit moet dus ook in ag geneem word by beoordeling.22 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 7 0 η. , . maggi van wyk om_te_ bepaal of die schwa-vnkggihy die eerste of enipste hprh»ii™ die beoordelmg was gegrond op die formantgebiede sowel as die ouditiewe beeld die koeffisient van die voorkoms van hierdie verskynsel by die verskillende kategoriee van elke proefpersoon afsonderlik en "ok die somtotaal van elke kategorie is bereken. " f e r i n e en ook g m j ^ p a a l o f d i e o p e e n v o l g e n d e h e r h a l i n g s v a n e l l ^ r r a ^ p ^ i n g n b e o a t o g j s i g t d l e v e r l a n g d e a r t i k u l a t o r i e s e ^ ^ h t ^ t f f ^ ook volgens n dne-punt skaal beoordeel, gegrond op die posisies van die spil (tweede formantbalk) van die herhalingsen vloteenhede. die spil verander die meeste in posisie en die spil van klanke koppel ook o p ' n bestendiger wyse. dit is dus die belangrikste formant om dop te hou by die identifisering van klankpatrone en kombinasies.22 die koeffisient van die voorkoms van hierdie verskynsel by die verskillende kategoriee van elke proefpersoon afsonderlik en ook die somtotaal van elke kategorie is bereken. deur die toepassing van 'n toets vir 'n verhouding is daar by al vy bogenoemde tabulerings bepaal of een van die kategoriee statisties betekenisvol verskil het van die ander. die waarde word jenoem die /z/resultate en bespreking tabel i bevat die ontleding van 315 spraakpogings, waarvan 41 (13%) geklassifiseer is as om van dieselfde duur te wees. by 274 (87 %) het die duur verskil van die van die ooreenstemmende vloteenheid. by 18 gevalle h v m ' ^ v a n , d i f h e r h a l i " g s e e n h e i d 16 31 msek. korter en oy μ u ι a ) i t 3i msek. langer as die van die vloteenheid. 107 gevalle het n duur van meer as 31 msek. korter en 114 (36 %) 'n duur van meer as 31 msek. langer getoon. 'n totaal van 70% d l ^ t w ^ d r i ' i % e k e n i s v 0 1 ° p d i e 5 % b e t e k e n i s ' p e i l geneem indien > i t 3 2 ? n i ? e h t e k e n i w o l o p ΐ 6 v 7 0 p e i l g e n e e m i n d i e n d i e waarde a 3,29. dit dui op n uiters betekenisvolle verskil. in die ontleding is 'n /zawaarde van 13,072 bereken, en daar is dus op 'n 0,1 % betekenlspeh gevind dat die woorddeelherhalings in tempo baie betekenisvol v s het van die ooreenstemmende vloteenhede. ook is gevind dat hierdie duurverskille groot was, aangesien die duurverskille by 70 % gevalle meer as 31 msek. was hoe groter die verskil in duur tussen die herhaltngsd i t t m o o n h w v l ° t e e n h e i d ' h o e opmerklik is die abnormalitei fn lu s t e r t " n w a a r ° m w o o r d d e e l h e r h a , i n g s uitgesonder word deur die luisteraar.2* daar is nie 'n statisties betekenisvolle verskil gevind tussen herhahngseenhede wat van korter en van langer duur was nie, aanges en n /zawaarde van 1,39 bereken is. montgomery en cooke2» skryf h!e die the south african journavof communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings 71 geval a β c d ε f g η i j κ l μ ν ο ρ q r van dieselfde duur < 8 msek 1 4 1 1 4 3 1 van korter duur 16-31 msek totaal persentasie gewys >31 msek 41 13% 18 6 % 22 4 1 3 6 5 4 4 4 3 2 3 24 8 2 5 3 4 van langer duur 16-31 msek 107 10 1 4 2 2 1 1 2 3 1 3 4 1 >31 msek 35 34% 24 3 3 6 17 7 1 2 3 1 6 6 6 6 13 3 7 114 11% 36% tabel ι 'n vergelyking van die tempo van die eerste herhalingsuiting met die uiting van die o o r e e n s t e m m e n d e vloteenheid. verskil toe aan die duur van die inisiele konsonant. hulle het. bevinc1 dat d s d u u r van hierdie konsonant van die gehakkelde woord 20 msek. of meer langer was as die van die ooreenstemmende konsonant van die z z o o r d hierdie bevindings stem ooreen met die hipotese wat aanvanklik gestel is. r^prpkinp van resultate soos getihii w r in tahelle ii. ill en iv bv"76~eevalle het twee herhalings per spraakpoging voorgekom 36 is d e u r d d e herhalings voorafgegaan en 17 deur vier. as gevolg van hierd.e t i a s i e moes die groepe aisonderlik ontleed w o r d ^ by d.e groepe ^ drie en vier herhalings per spraakpoging is die verskil bepaal tussen die met die laneste en die met die kortste duur. . b y d i e ^praakpogings wat deur twee sowel j ^ r d n e woorddeelherhalings voorafgegaan is, is daar op die 0,1 £ ^ sevind dat daar 'n beduidende duurverskil t u s s e n d . e herhahngseenhede vooreekom het. by eg. is 'n /z/-waarde van 8,603 bereken en by lg. 5,833. d a a r t s e g t e r n i e 'n statisties betekenisvolle verskil verkry tussen die s t a l e aanfal wat van korter duur was en die wat van langer duur was nie. ^ ^ ^ e mln gevalle voorgekom het waar vier herhalings die vlotuiting voorafgegaan het, o m statisties te ontleed, blyk dit dat dieselfde neiging as by die vorige gevalle voorkom. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 maggi van wyk geval van dieselfde duur msek d 16-31 korter uur msek langer d 34-5i korter uur msek langer d 62,5-'; korter uur 8 msek langer d >78 korter uur msek langer a β c d ε f g η i j κ l μ ν 0 ρ 0 r 2 1 1 1 1 1 2 2 1 1 2 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 1 3 6 2 1 1 1 1 1 4 3 1 5 2 5 3 3 1 3 1 totaal 0 4 11 7 2 5 3 21 23 /ο 0% 5% 15% 9% 3% 7% 4% 28% 30% tabel ii 'n vergelyking van die duur van woorddeelherhalings by gevalle waar daar twee herhalings per spraakpoging voorgekom het. gevan dieduur duur duur ^ duur geselfde 16-31 msek 34-55 msek 62,5-78 msek > 78 msek val duur korlangkorlangkorlangkorlangί 8 msek ter er ter er ter er ter er a 1 1 3 4 β 1 1 c d 1 1 2 1 ε • 1 1 1 2 5 f g η 1 1 i 1 j j 1 κ 1 l μ 1 / 1 ν 2 0 • 1 ρ 1 0 r to0 0 1 taal 0 0 1 1 3 4 2 12 13 0/ /ο 0% 0% 3% 3% 8% 11% 6% 33% 36% tabel iii 'n vergelyking van die duur van woorddeelherhalings by gevalle waar daar drie herhalings per spraakpoging voorgekom het. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings geval a β c d ε f g η i j κ l μ ν 0 ρ 0 r van die selfde duur < 8 msek du 16-31 korter ur msek langer du 34-55 korter ur msek langer du 62,5-78 korter ur msek langer du >78 korter ur msek langer geval a β c d ε f g η i j κ l μ ν 0 ρ 0 r 1. 2 3 •1 7 1 1 1 totaal 1 2 4 10 0/ /ο 6% 12% 24% 59%· tabel iv 'n vergelyking van die duur van woorddeelherhalings by gevalle waar daar vier herhalings per spraakpoging voorgekom het. uit tabelle ii en iii blyk dit dat by die meeste gevalle nl. 58 % en 69 % gevalle onderskeidelik, die duurverskil meer as 78 msek. was. by alle gevalle was daar 'n duurverskil van meer as 16 msek. die veelvuldige herhalings van hakkelaars word dus opmerklik onreelmatig geuiter, wat van riper se kliniese indruk van hakkelspraak bevestig i.t.m. die normale onvlothede van nie-hakkelaars wat glad, reelmatig en teen dieselfde tempo as die res van hul lettergrepe geuiter word.28 montgomery en cooke het ook by 62 % van pare monsters wat hulle ontleed het η verskil in tempo van produksie opgemerk.20 dit bevestig ook die hipotese wat aanvanklik gestel is. die voorkoms al dan nie van die verlangde oorgangsfornimtbyjie^gmg of enipste woorddeelherhaling wat die vlotuiting voorafgaan (sien tabel daar is gevind dat die verlangde oorgangsformant by 30% gevalle wel voorgekom het, by 64% was dit onwaarskynlik en by 6 h was die moontlikheid nie uitgesluit dat dit wel die verlangde oorgang was me statisties is op die 0,1 % betekenispeil gevind dat dit onwaarskynlik is dat die oorgangsformant van die eerste of enigste woorddeelherhaling ooreenstem met die verlangde formant van die vloteenheid.-aangesien η /z/-waarde van 5,99 bereken is. dit bevestig van riper se hipotese. hierdie bevinding word ook nog onderskryf deur waarnemings van die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 maggi van wyk geval ja moontlik onwaarskynlik a 4 3 58 β 3 1 9 c 1 0 7 d 1 0 12 ε 5 0 24 f 6 2 4 g 4 0 3 η 9 0 4 i 1 2 8 j 1 1 3 κ ' 1 1 1 l 1 0 10 μ 32 0 15 ν 8 2 6 ο 0 1 8 ρ 10 3 11 0 4 1 9 r 3 1 9 totaal 94 18 201 % 30% 6% 64% tabel v die voorkoms van die verlangde oorgangsformant by die eerste of enigste woorddeelherhaling wat die vlotuiting voorafgaan. agnello1 en montgomery en cooke.2 0 dit is dus moontlik dat die formantafwykings reflekteer dat die konsonant van die gehakkelde woord geproduseer was met 'n abnormale stand van die artikulasie organe. daarom is die artikulatoriese (en dus akoestiese) verloop vanaf hierdie stand na die nodig vir die produksie van die verlangde vokaal ooreenstemmend versteur. die outeurs hipotetiseer dus dat die abnormale formantoorgange die gevolg is van afwykende artikulasie van die konsonant eerder as wat dit die gevolg is van foutiewe oorgangsdinamika.20 daar word ook weer verwys na die bevindinge van stromsta,25 aangaande die gebrek aan formantoorgange as diagnostiese middel vir die identifisering van hakkelaars. hierdie bevinding bevestig die hipotese wat aanvanklik gestel is. aangaande die gebruik van die schwa-vokaal deur hakkelaars by hul woorddeelherhalings. daar is gevind dat by 22 % gevalle die schwa-vokaal wel geproduseer is, wat goed ooreenstem met montgomery en cooke se bevinding van 25 %.20 verder was dit by 51 % van gevalle onwaarskynlik, terwyl daar by 27 % wel die moontlikheid bestaan het dat die schwa geproduseer is. statistics is op die 0,1 % betekenispeil gevind dat dit onwaarskynlik is dat die schwa-vokaal gebruik is by die eerste of enigste woorddeelherhalings van hierdie hakkelaars. hierdie bevinding is teenstrydig met die verwagte resultate asook met van riper se kliniese indruk:2" almost universally the schwa vowel can be heard in the stutterer's abortive speech attempts. hierdie teenstrydigheid kan verklaar word aan die hand van die feit dat the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings 75 geval ja moontlik onwaarskynlik a 21 15 28 β 2 2 7 c 3 2 2 d 4 1 8 ε 11 11 4 f 2 3 1 g 0 1 6 η 0 2 11 i 4 1 5 i .2 0 2 κ 1 .1 0 l 3 1 6 μ 2 12 30 ν 1 5 8 ο 1 4 4 ρ 4 6 14 0 2 6 3 r 1 4 8 totaal 64 77 147 /ο 22% 27% 51% t a b e l vi 'n beoordeling van die voorkoms van die schwa-vokaal b y die eerste of enigste woorddeelherhalings van spraakpogings waar 'n neutrale vokaal nie verlang word nie. geval ja moontlik onwaarskynlik a 2 0 4 β 0 0 5 c 0 0 3 · d 0 0 4 ε 0 0 4 f 0 0 4 g 1 0 0 η 1 0 3 i 0 2 2 j 0 1 2 κ 0 0 1 l 0 0 1 μ 0 0 5 ν 1 0 2 ο 0 0 4 ρ 0 1 2 0 1 0 2 r 0 0 0 totaal 6 4 48 /ο 10% 7% 83% t a b e l vii die voorkoms van benaderingsgedrag. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 maggi van wyk die vorige bewerings grootliks gebaseer was op kliniese indrukke as 'n deeglike analise. hierdie indrukke reflekteer die natuurlike neiging van 'n luisteraar wanneer hy vokaalsegmente van kort duur teenkom, soos by deelwoordherhalings of oor die algemeen wanneer onsekerheid teengekom word, om tot die gevolgtrekking te kom dat die neutrale vokaal geuiter is.20 die voorkoms van benaderingsgedrag by 83% het dit geblyk dat dit onwaarskynlik is dat daar van benaderingsgedrag sprake is. by 69 % het definitiewe benaderingsgedrag voorgekom en by 7 % gevalle was dit moontlik. t.s.v. die relatief min spraakpogings met genoeg woorddeelherhalings om hierdie eienskap te ondersoek, soos die geval met montgomery en cooke 2 0 se ondersoek, is daar statisties op die 0,1 % betekenispeil gevind dat dit onwaarskynlik is dat die veelvuldige herhalings 'n wyse van benadering tot die verlangde oorgang van die vlotuiting is. hierdie bevinding korreleer dus nie met die hipotese wat gestel is nie en ook nie met van riper se kliniese indruk nie.28 gevolgtrekkings buiten die voorkomsfrekwensie van onderbroke woorde, is daar ook nog ander eienskappe waarvolgens 'n diagnose van hakkel gemaak kan word. die bevinding van hierdie studie is dat die tempo waarteen hakkelherhalings geuiter word verskil van die van normale onvlothede en ook dat eg. opmerklik onreelmatig is. kliniese waarnemings het getoon dat tydsberekening 'n belangrike faktor is in die voorkoms van hakkel. so bv. bei'nvloed tydsdruk die hakkelaar se spraak. die hakkelaar vertoon soekende gedrag-gedrag wat daarop gerig is om die regte tydsberekening vir woorduiting te bewerkstellig.26 dit is dus moontlik dat hakkel ontstaan as gevolg van 'n biologiese onvermoe om op morfemiese vlak motoriese patrone temporaal te bemeester. aangesien daar geen reelmatigheid ten opsigte van tempoverskille voorgekom het nie, word die indruk geskep dat hierdie gedrag onwillekeurig is, soos webster en brutten dit ook stel.30 hoewel daar gevind is dat die verlangde oorgangsformante nie by die woorddeelherhalings van hakkelaars voorkom nie, is dit egter ook nie die schwa-vokaal wat gebruik word nie. die veelvuldige herhalings is ook nie 'n poging tot benadering van die verlangde oorgang nie. die artikulatoriese model voorgestel deur henke1 9 kan moontlik gebruik/word om die abnormale temporale aspek en die ongewensde oorgangsformant waarmee die hakkelspasma gei'nisieer word, met mekaar in verband te bring. die toevoer van die model is 'n string foon-grootte segmente wat elk gespesifiseer is i.t.v. 'n stel artikulatoriese mikpunte. laasgenoemde stel 'n beperkte reeks saam wat onderskeidelik gevarieer word in tyd. hulle is vorme en posisies van die articulators. hoewel die artikulators na hierdie spesifieke mikpunte beweeg, mag hulle moontlik nie bereik word nie. die posisies wat bereik word hang af van die artikulasiestande wat the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) / oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings 77 voorheen deur die artikulators ingeneem is en van die tydreeling van opeenvolgende toevoerings. henke stel ook 'n vooruitbeskouing van toekomstige toevoereenhede voor, wat 'n verwagting van koartikulasie van sekere eienskappe toelaat, wanneer dit nie in konflik is met die gespesifiseerde mikpunte van meer onmiddellike eenhede nie. daar kan dus tot die gevolgtrekking gekom word dat woorddeelherhalings nie alleen 'n essensiele eienskap is wat algemeen by hakkelaars opgemerk word nie,2 8 maar dat dit ook sekere distinktiewe eienskappe bevat wat veroorsaak dat die luisteraar dit juis uitsonder as hakkelgedrag. d i e w a a r d e v a n d i e s t u d i e e n m o o n t l i k e a a n b e v e l i n g s spraakkenmerke wat van belang is vir die differensiaaldiagnose tussen hakkelspraak en normale onvlothede is deur hierdie studie bevestig. naamlik dat woorddeelherhalings onreelmatig geuiter word en nie teen dieselfde tempo as die vlotuiting nie, en ook die afwesigheid van die verlangde oorgangsformant by die inisiele herhaling. hierdie was vroeer slegs kliniese indrukke. 2 8 ' 2 5 dit impliseer terapeuties dat daar met die afbraak van die hierargiese gedragsvorme van hakkelaars begin moet word by die willekeurige sekondere gedrag. aangesien die primere gedragsvorme onwillekeurig is,30 mag die volgende tegnieke van waarde wees: tegnieke wat ontspanning meebring onder andere johnson se bonstegniek, verlengde spraak en ligte kontakte deur die artikulators.1' metodes wat die hakkelaar met die tydsberekening van spraakpogings help, kan ook met sukses in terapie aangewend w o r d . 2 ' 3 ' 2 7 die volgende aspekte mag interessant wees om verder na te vors: aangesien dit nie die schwa is wat oor die algemeen deur hakkelaars gebruik word in hulle inisiele herhalings nie, kan dit van waarde wees om te bepaal watter klank dan wel die meeste gebruik word. navorsing word benodig aangaande verdere aspekte van belang vir differensiaaldiagnose28 nl: of lugvloei by hakkelherhalings wel onderbreek word en of skielike fonatoriese stakings dit vergesel; of pouses by normale lettergreepherhalings baie korter is as die van gehakkelde herhalings. daar kan ook nog dieper ingegaan word op die kwessie van benadering. dit mag interessant wees om te bepaal of die fonetiese samestelling van die vloteenheid 'n rol speel by die korter of langer duur van die herhalingseenheid. daar is bevind dat daar wel tot duidelike resultate en gevolgtrekkings gekom kan word deur 'n enkele komponente gedragsvorm van die konglomeraat te selekteer en dit te analiseer.30 daar is ook bevind dat spektrografie nie alleenlik 'n waardevolle hulpmiddel is by eksperimentele ondersoek nie, maar dat dit ook van diagnostiese belang is om spektrografiese ontledings van spraakmonsters te maak. sodoende word spraakkenmerke van spraakafwykings uitgelig, wat nie bloot deur die ouditiewe bepaal kan word nie. hierdie siening word gesteun en bevestig deur stromsta,25 van riper,28 montgomery en cooke 2 0 en potter, kopp en kopp.2 2 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 maggi van wyk verwysings 1. agnello, j.g. (1975): measurement procedures in speech, hearing and language, university park press. 2. barber, v. (1939): chorus reading as a distraction in stuttering, studies in the psychology of stuttering xv, j. speech dis., 4,371-383. 3. barber, v. (1940): rhythm as a distraction in stuttering, studies in the psychology of stuttering xvi, j. speech dis., 15, 29-42. 4. barr, h. (1940): a quantitative study of the specific phenomena observed in stuttering, j. speech dis., 5, 277-280. 5. beech, h.r. en fransella, f. (1966): research and experiment in stuttering, pergamon press, oxford. 6. bloodstein, o. (1961): the development of stuttering iii. theoretical and clinical implications, j. speech hear. dis., 26, nr. 1, 67-81. 7. boehmler, r.m. (1958): listener responses to non-fluencies, j. speech hear. res., 1, 132-141. 8. brutten, g. en shoemaker, d. (1971): a two factor learning theory of stuttering. in handbook of speech pathology and audiology, travis, l. (ed.). appleton century crofts, new york, bis. 1035 1072. 9. dalton, p. en hardcastle, w.j. (1977): disorders of fluency and their effects on communication, billing and sons ltd london. 10. davis, d.m. (1939): the relation of repetition in the speech of young children to certain measures of language maturity and situational factors, part i, j. speech dis., 4, 308-318. 11. flanagan, j.l. (1972): speech analysis synthesis and perception, 2nd edit. universitatsdriickerei, wiirzburg. 12. froeschels, e. (1952): the significance of symptomatology for the understanding of the essence of stuttering, folia foniatrica, iv, 217-230. 13. helmreich, h.g. en bloodstein, o. (1973): the grammatical factor in childhood disfluency in relation to the continuity hypothesis, j. speech hear. res., 16, 731-738. 14. jakobson, r., fant, g. en halle, m. (1963): preliminaries to speech analysis: the distinctive features and their correlates, m.i.t. press. 15. johnson, w., darley, f.l. en spriestersbach, d.c. (1963): diagnostic methods in speech pathology, harper & row, new york. 16. kopp, g. en green, h. (1948): visible speech, volta'review, feb., 60-62. 17. kopp, g.a., kopp, h.g., angelocci, a. (1967): visible speech manual, wayne state university press, detroit. 18. minifee, f.d. en cooker, h.s. (1964): a dysfluency index, j. speech hear. dis., xxix, 189-193. , 19. moll, k.l. en daniloff, r.g. (1971): investigation of the timing of velar movements during speech, part 2, j. acoustical soc. america, 50, nr. 2. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) oudiovisuele analise van woorddeelherhalings 79 20. montgomery, a.a. en cooke, p.a. (1976): perceptual and acoustical analysis of repetitions in stuttered speech, j. commun. dis., 9, 317330. 21. peters, r. en giles, m. (1965): stuttering in relation to syntactical and phonemic structure of the english language, kongresverslag van die internasionale vereniging vir logopedie en foniatrie, 11, 321-323. 22. potter, r.k., kopp, g.a., kopp, h.g. (1966): visible speech, dover publications inc., new york. 23. sander, e.k. (1963): frequency of syllable repetition and stutter judgements, j. speech hear. res., vi, 19-30. 24. sheehan, j.g. (1974): stuttering behaviour: a phonetic analysis., j. commun. dis., 7, 193-212. 25. stromsta, c. (1965): a spectrographic study of dysfluencies labelled as stuttering by parents, kongresverslag van die internasionale vereniging van logopedie en foniatrie, ii, 317-318. 26. uys, i.c. (1973): 'n biologies-gefundeerde hipotese in verband met die ontstaan van hakkel, tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, 20, des. 47-58. 27. van dantzig, m. (1940): syllable-tapping, a new method for the help of stammerers, j. speech dis., 5, 127-131. 28. van riper, c. (1971): the nature of stuttering, prentice hall inc. 29. van riper, c. (1972): speech correction: principles and methods, prentice-hall inc. 5th edit. 30. webster, l. en brutten, g. (1972): an audiovisual behavioral analysis of the stuttering moment, behaviour therapy, 3, 555-560. 31. wingate, m.e. (1976): stuttering theory and treatment, irvington publishers inc., new york. 32. wingate, m.e. (1964): a standard definition of stuttering, j. speech hear. dis., xxix, 484-489. 33. wingate, m.e. (1969): stuttering as phonetic transition defect, j. speech hear. dis., xxxiv, 107-108. 34. yairi, e. en clifton, n. (1972): dysfluent speech behaviour of preschool children, high school seniors, and geriatric persons, j. speech hear. res., 15, 714-719. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aids for • the development of perception • the acquisition of speech and language skills • the improvement of motor co-ordination plus • helpful texts for therapists • educational toys, books and equipment • records for auditory training • catalogues on request • large variety of tests available new arrivals: • learning to listen • two sound lottos • "listen what is that?" play and schoolroom 8 tyrwhitt avenue, rosebank (adjoining the constantia cinema) telephones: 788-1304 p.o. box 52137, saxonwold, 2132 ο m the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aspects of cohesion, tense and pronoun usage in the discourse of the older language-impaired child hilary berger, b.a. (sp. & h. th.) (witwatersrand) speech therapy department, transvaal memorial hospital for children, johannesburg. aletta sinoff, b.a. (sp. & h. th.) (witwatersrand) department speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary aspects of the discourse of 5 language-impaired children and 5 children with no language impairment, aged approximately 9 years, were compared. a film and a story sequence were utilised to elicit narratives on which, measures of cohesion, tense and pronouns were appraised. measures of cohesion refer to the ability to indicate appropriately the relations of meaning with regard to situational context. measures of tense include aspects of tense range and tense continuity. measures of pronouns refer to the anaphoric use of pronouns with non-ambiguous referents. the group of language-impaired children was found to be significantly poorer on measures of cohesion and pronominal usage than the normal children, whereas a significant difference between the two groups was not revealed on measures of tense. possible factors accounting for these findings were discussed and' implications for the diagnosis and therapy of the older language-impaired child were considered. opsomming aspekte van die uitinge van 5 taalversteurde kinders en 5 kinders sonder taalversteuring van ongeveer 9 jaar, is vergelyk. 'n film en 'n storie-reeks is gebruik om 'n vertelling te ontlok waarvolgens maatstawwe van kohesie, tyd en voornaamwoorde geevalueer is. maatstawwe van kohesie verwys na die vermoe om die verhouding van betekenis met betrekking tot situasie konteks toepaslik aan te dui. maatstawwe van tydsvorme sluit in aspekte van die omvang van tydsvorme en die aaneenskakeling van tydsvorme. maatstawwe van voornaamwoorde verwys na die anaforiese gebruik van voornaamwoorde met ondubbelsinnige verwysings. die groep taalversteurde kinders het beduidend swakker presteer ten opsigte van maatstawwe van kohesie en voornaamwoord gebruik as die normale kinders terwyl geen beduidende verskil uitgewys is tussen die twee groepe kinders met betrekking tot tydsvorme me. moontlike faktore verantwoordelik vir hierdie bevindinge is bespreek en imphkasies vir diagnose en terapie van die ouer taalversteurde kind is oorweeg. • recent research in child language has indicated the need to view pragmatics. this has highlighted the importance of accounting for 'reallife'., communication situations, for example, the appropriateness of utterances with reference to the context in which they are produced and understood. 6 ' 2 0 ' 24<28 application of this to the field of language pathology suggests a number of implications for the diagnosis of and therapy for language disorders. an examination of aspects of pragmatics is afforded by discourse, that is, the sentences spoken or written in succession by one or more persons in a single situation.14 the concept of cohesion may be considered as central to discourse. it refers to the relation of meaning expressed in discourse and the appropriate linking of these relations.12 semantic cohesion is described as die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, j978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 η. berger & α. sinoff characterizing the 'backbone' of narratives and is formed by appropriately relating the chain of events.'4 syntactic cohesion refers to the linguistic forms used to indicate appropriately the interrelationships between utterances and draws partly from the syntax and partly from the vocabulary.12 description of the language of the older child, that is, the child from five years of age, is limited.22 research in this area has focused on the understanding and production of isolated sentences with an emphasis on the adequacy of syntactic form and to some extent semantic a s p e c t s . 9 ' i 0 ' 1 7 however, language beyond the single sentence within a context has received less attention. the importance of accounting for these aspects is aptly stated by cazden:8 a child's manifest verbal behaviour or performance has both grammatical and pragmatic aspects. thus, a consideration of discourse becomes particularly relevant in examining the child's communicative ability. the discourse of the older clinical child deserves attention, as it has been stated that these children lack the ability to . . . talk coherently and to the point.24 a study conducted by t u c h 2 7 investigated this aspect in the narratives of a group of 9-year old language-impaired and normal speaking children. results revealed the language-impaired group's failure to convey logicality resulting in incoherent narratives. thus the clinical child may be competent on a sentence level, but may manifest difficulty with the expression of inter-sentence relations in discourse. two syntactic forms which rely on inter-sentence relations in discourse, and have relevance for the clinical child, are tense and pronouns. research on both these aspects has dealt largely with the acquisition p r o c e s s . 7 ' i 5 ' , 8 ' l 9 ' 2 1 ' 2 9 with reference to tense, emerging linguistic forms indicating the relationship of events in time have been viewed together with the child's cognitive development.'' 4 ' 5 a consideration of the scope of tense reveals that the linguistic forms of the present and past tense are closely related to aspect, that is, the view of the verbal action either as in progress — the progressive aspect, or as completed — the perfect aspect.2 5 this results in a number of complex tense forms. these tenses are acquired later than the simple tenses in the developmental sequence.4 only a small proportion of studies in this area make reference to the older child. in terms of acquisition of tense use, it appears that from approximately six to seven years of age, the child's ability is comparable to that of the adult.5 this however does not account for the specific handling of tense beyond the single sentence level, for example, the reporting of direct speech in discourse. hendricks14 uses the term "intersentence concord" to refer to the consistent use of the same tense of the main verbs in the sentences comprising a narrative. this affords an examination of tense continuity or the extent of tense shifts. research on pronouns has indicated) that the child of nine years has mastered the various features of pronoun selection on a sentence level, such as person, gender and number, as well as the grammatical rules that the pronominalization process is subject t o . 1 8 ' 1 9 ' 2 9 however there has the south african journal of communication disorders. vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aspects of discourse in language impairment 5 been little research carried out on the use of pronouns in discourse aspects of pragmatics such as role-playing have to be taken into consideration in a discourse situation where, for example, a child is required to realize that if there are two male participants in a narrative pronominalization of both after the initial naming will cause confusion aspects of pronoun usage in discourse which contribute to cohesion are anaphoric pronouns which function as replacements for co-referential noun phrases (np's) in preceding clauses. the pronoun as a reference item functions as a deictic, that is, it refers to an np or to knowledge shared by the speaker and listener.'6 for cohesive purposes the identification of the pronoun must be specific. for example, "john went to town. he bought shoes". the pronoun "he" is an anaphoric pronoun as it replaces the np "john"; or; "for he is a jolly good fellow". in this case the speaker and listener should be able to identify "he" from their mutual knowledge of the context for the identification of "he" to be specific. application of the measurement and characterization of aspects of discourse has received little attention. thus the present report aims to examine the older language-impaired child's discourse, and proposes parameters which are relevant for such an analysis. methodology aim to examine aspects of the communicative ability as revealed by discourse, of a group of older language-impaired children and to compare the communicative ability of this group with that of non language-impaired children of the same age range. specifically, to compare the two groups on the following measures:measures of cohesion the ability to relate the chain of events such that the relation of meaning is expressed.1 2'1 4 this involves a consideration of appropriateness to situational context, the logicality and sequence with which events are recounted, as well as fluency. measures of tense the appropriate use of complex tenses, as well as tense range and tense continuity. measures of pronouns the anaphoric use of third person pronouns with non-ambiguous referents. subjects two groups of subjects (ss) comprising an experimental (e) group of 5 language-impaired children and a control (c) group of 5 non languageimpaired children were used. age range was from 8,4 to 9,6 years. the ε ss had language problems predominantly of an expressive nature as assessed by the speech therapists attached to the two remedial schools from which the ss were selected. ss were receiving speech therapy at the time of testing. all ss came from white english-speaking, middle-class south african homes. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 η. berger & α. sinoff ss in the ε and c groups met the following criteria for subject selection:(1) age:the older child was selected as, according to piaget,23 there is a decline in egocentric language from 8 years of age, and children begin to be more explicit in their narrative. this aspect was relevant for the study of pronouns in discourse and cohesion. secondly, by 8 years of age, features of tense have been acquired on the single sentence level,5 but the extent of competence in discourse has not been explored. (2) intelligence:the intelligence of all ss was considered to be within normal limits. for the ε group, iq scores on the new south african individual scale were obtained from the case-files at the respective remedial schools. exact scores were unavailable for the c group, however all c ss were judged by their teachers to be of average intelligence. t a s k s the data to be analysed was derived from the retelling of a film or story sequence. this was felt to be a close approximation to a 'real-life' discourse situation. also, children tend to talk more when presented with this type of stimulus as compared with pictures or a toy.8 the following tasks were therefore administered :(1) one sequence (1 minute 15 seconds) was selected from a film of the "three stooges", and presented audio-visually. in terms of investigating the child's anaphoric use of third person pronouns (he; him; they; them; it) with non-ambiguous referents, the film sequence was required to have more than 2 male actors with equal interaction. this was aimed at creating a potentially ambiguous situation where there were 2 or more referents of the same sex. (2) one story sequence presented auditorily. this task was devised particularly for analysis of tense use. results from a pilot study carried out by the writers revealed that a film sequence (such as that described in (1) above) required relating events that have occurred in the immediate past, and therefore would not be sufficiently taxing for assessing complex tense use. thus a story sequence was devised which contained some later developing conjunctions,1 8 for example, since; when; while; unless; if; after; before, and temporal adverbs — all of which require the use of more complex tenses. the story was tape-recorded to ensure that presentation to each subject was consistent. / to control for memory span, the number of events in the film and story sequence was limited to 7. p r o c e d u r e i all ss were tested individually by 2 experimenters (the writers e l and e2). testing was carried out in 2 rooms — 1 room where the film and story sequences were presented by el and a second room, where the narratives the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aspects of discourse in language impairment 7 were recorded by a 'naive' listener (e2). the following is an explanation of the procedure followed for each s: (1) a period of casual conversation between the s and experimenters preceded the presentation of tasks. this was aimed at creating a relaxed atmosphere in an attempt to control for the influence of the testing situation on communication. (2) the instructions for the film and story tasks included the information that the s was retelling the sequences to a listener who was unfamiliar with the content (e2). this was aimed at approximating a 'real-life' situation which requires that the speaker be explicit rather than rely on the listener's previous knowledge of the content of the sequences. (3) e l presented the film first, followed by the story. after the presentation, the s was required to retell the narrative to e2 who had not been present during the task presentation. a methodological point of significance was the use o f " . . . tell m e . . " as part of the instructions to each subject. this prevented the influence of the tense used in the instructions on the tense of the s's narratives.5 during the s's discourse, e2 acknowledged his comments with the expression of "mm" and nodding of the head so as to approximate a natural speaker-listener effect. the s's narratives were tape-recorded and transcribed on both tasks the scoring system devised by the writers was then applied to the protocols measures of cohesion, tense and pronouns were analysed on the film task; measures of cohesion and tense (but not pronouns) were analysed on the story task. measuring and scoring system. the measures of aspects of communicative ability may be divided into 3 main sections following the aim of the study. (i) measures of cohesion (ii) measures of tense (iii) measures of pronouns each of these measures and the scoring system used is outlined below. (4) (5) (i) measures of cohesion this represents an appraisal of the ability to relate appropriately the chain of events such that the relation of meaning is expressed 12>'< measures of this ability were divided as follows:(a) semantic content this was measured by:(a) number of main events. the film and story, sequences were each divided into 7 main events. the number of events reported by each s on each task was tallied. the requirement was that the event be mentioned irrespective of its position in the narrative or its grammaticality. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 η. berger & α. sinoff (b) number of inaccuracies of content. this was felt to detract from the general cohesiveness of the narrative. the number of inaccurately reported events on each task was tallied. (b) semantic cohesion (a) sequencing of events and logicality. this measure was derived from piaget's work2 3 on the "egocentric" language characterizing children's narratives. this measure refers to the ability to express appropriately casual relations, that is, the "how" of events; the ability to order events in the correct time sequence; and the ability to recount all parts of a narrative into a meaningful whole. this was measured by:(1) the number of events related logically and in sequence ( + log seq). the number of events related logically and in sequence in each narrative were tallied as (+ log seq). the following example is an extract from a c s's narrative to illustrate this measure:"it was about a cow and they're trying to milk it and they're using its tail and it didn't give milk . . . " (2) the number of errors in logic and sequence (— log seq). the number of errors in logic and sequence in each narrative were tallied as (— log seq). errors were evident where events were listed without any form of logical, temporal or casual connection such that no interrelationship was implied, and where narratives lacked specificity and events were omitted. the following example is an extract from an ε s's narrative to illustrate this measure:"then they try, they milk the cow — so, so then on it was a hole in one side and went into his face". with regard to both (1) and (2), the ability to express the semantic relations of the original story was considered together with the use of appropriate syntactic forms in the expression of these relations. (b) fluency. this measure was based on the categories applied by haynes and hood 1 3 when analysing fluency changes as a function of linguistic complexity. these were: interjections; part-word, word and phrase repetitions; revisions; incomplete phrases and tense pauses. the number of times when dysfluency was felt to affect the cohesiveness of the narrative was tallied. this was determined according to a subjective evaluation by e l and e2. from the measures of semantic content and semantic cohesion, a general appraisal of cohesion was obtained. protocols were scored independently by the writers, and cotcurrence was reached in the majority of instances. where there was a discrepancy, the writers reviewed the narratives. ( i i ) measures of tense (a) tense range this was measured by:| (a) the number of complex tenses used appropriately. the number of appropriate present and past forms, of the progressive, perfect and the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aspects of discourse in language impairment 9 perfect progressive were tallied. in addition, the variety of present and past tense forms was qualitatively examined. the following example is taken from a c s's narrative on the story task and illustrates the appropriate use of the present and past progressive tenses: "sam's class was going to the zoo. they were very excited. when sam came into the class, the teacher said to him: "why are you so dirty". sam said: "while i was playing soccer, another boy tripped me . . ." (b) tense continuity this was measured by:(a) the number of inappropriate tense shifts of a main verb. this measure determined the extent of tense shifting before or after the reporting of direct speech. a score was obtained by tallying the number of inappropriate tense shifts of a main verb. the following example is taken from a c s's narrative to illustrate this: ". . . and he did it about three times and nothing happened. so he says: "try again", and then it went into his face, and he says: "that's a nice flavour". the verb "says" in both instances is in the present tense instead of the past tense. from a consideration of aspects of complex tense use and tense continuity, a measure of tense in discourse was obtained. ( i l l ) measures of pronouns (a) ambiguous pronoun usage this was measured by:(a) the number of nonindexed pronouns — that is, where there was no referent corresponding to the pronouns. the number of nonindexed pronouns was tallied. the following example is taken from an ε s's narrative to illustrate this measure:"when he brought a cow into the house and he wanted to milk it so he didn't know . . ." one is unable to determine to whom the "he" refers. (b) ambiguous pronouns that is, where the identity of the referent corresponding to the pronoun was ambiguous. the number of instances when this occurred was tallied. the following example is taken from an ε s's narrative to illustrate this measure: "the man went flying... and then the other man said: 'i'll show you how'. . . . and then he took the long . . . then he pulled, then he said: "what's wrong". there are two possible referents for "he", that is, 'the man' and 'the other man', resulting in ambiguity. (c). incorrect reference of the pronoun that is, where identity of the referent was incorrect. this was based on the experimenters' knowledge of the content of the narrative. from a consideration of each of the above measures, a general measure of ambiguous pronoun usage in discourse was obtained. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 η. berger & α. sinoff statistical treatment of data due to the 'free production' nature of the discourse tasks, the ε and c groups' narratives could not be directly compared.2 6 thus a co-variate for each task had to be identified. the co-variate selected was the number of words on the film task, and the number of sentences on the story task. the spearman rank order correlation test was used to correlate each of the measures with the co-variate. where correlations were significant, i.e., where the co-variate influenced the scores obtained, the scores were divided by the co-variate. a mann whitney u test for significance was then carried out to compare the ε and c groups on each measure. results a significant difference between the ε and c groups was found on measures of semantic cohesion and ambiguous pronoun usage. the groups did not differ significantly on measures of semantic content, tense range and tense continuity (table i). measures of cohesion film task story task semantic content number of main events number of inaccuracies 7,5 8,5 4,5 8 semantic cohesion ( + log seq) ( l o g s e q ) fluency 3 x 3 x 6 1 x 3 x 2,5 x measures of tense tense range the number of complex tenses used appropriately 11,5 10 tense continuity the number of inappropriate tense shifts of a main verb 11 5 measures of pronouns / / measures of ambiguous pronoun usage 4 x not analysed values represent the mann whitney u scores obtained on each measure. x — significant differences between the ε and c groups (p = 0,05). (+ logseq) — the number of events related logically and in sequence. (— log seq) — the number of errors in logic and sequence. ι table 1. summary table of significant differences between the ε and c groups on measures of cohesion, tense and pronouns. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aspects of discourse in language impairment 11 d i s c u s s i o n o f r e s u l t s measures of cohesion on measures of semantic content, the ε and c groups did not differ significantly, demonstrating the ability to include the relevant features in their narratives. the significant difference between the groups on measures of cohesion related to the manner in which the events were related. the ε groups' narratives were characterized by more errors affecting logic and general sequence. with this group events were merely listed resulting in incoherent narratives which the ss were unable to communicate effectively. in contrast, the c group scored higher on the number of events related logically and in sequence, thus producing coherent stories. contributing to the ε ss' incoherence, was their dysfluency in relating the narratives. this created discontinuity and reduced the speaker's communicative efficiency. measures of tense from table i it can be seen that the ε ss did not experience difficulty with aspects of tense use in discourse. however with regard to aspects of tense range, a qualitative difference between the two groups was found. this manifested in the variety and range of simple and complex tenses used. for example, all ss utilised simple tenses, however the ε ss used fewer complex tenses than the c ss. generally the ε ss used a small variety of tense forms, specifically in terms of complex tense usage. for example, no ε s used the present progressive, present perfect, and perfect progressive tenses, although these were used by all c ss. results on tense continuity revealed a small number of tense shifts characterizing the narratives of both groups. an in-depth analysis of the narratives revealed that all instances of tense shift of a main verb occurred either preceding or following the reporting of direct speech. this was illustrated by the substitution of present tense for past tense. for example: " . . . and some stones fell on top of his head and then the fat one says: 'try again' and then another man came over . . ." a consideration of the appropriateness of the tasks employed for measuring tense range and tense continuity indicated that the story task was more revealing of aspects of tense range than the film task. it seems that this was related to the presence of temporal conjunctions and adverbs ('since' and 'while' in the example below) in the story task which implied complex tense usage. this is illustrated in the following example taken from a c s's narrative on the story task: "sam's class wanted to go to the zoo and they had been looking forward to it since the beginning of the term . . . his teacher said: 'why are your clothes so dirty'. so he said: 'while i was playing soccer somebody came and tripped me . . ." with regard to tense continuity, the film task was more appropriate than the story task. since the film contained direct speech only, ss tended to die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 η. berger & α. sinoff recount as much of this as possible. this therefore provided the context for tense shifts to occur. measures of pronouns a significant difference between the ε and c groups was found on measures of ambiguous pronoun usage. the ε ss used more non-indexed pronouns in their narratives than the c ss, that is, the ε ss violated the rule of referring to an np and appeared to rely on presupposed mutual knowledge shared by the speaker and listener. for example: "you see, he was sitting on the floor, and then he said that, then he said: 'wait a minute' — because he wanted to try and get him up". both groups demonstrated a certain amount of difficulty with explicit reference to pronouns. where there were two possible referents for a pronoun, ss did not use the pronouns specifically. for example: "the one man was sitting on the floor and the other tried to get him onto the bed and they were trying to get it out and he comes and he cuts their trousers and he hit him and he went flying onto the top bunk." g e n e r a l d i s c u s s i o n . from the above results, it is apparent that the ε group's discourse was inferior to that of the c group, in that the ε group's narratives demonstrated poor semantic cohesion and restricted syntactic forms in comparison to the c group. it is interesting to note'that restricted forms were seen not only in terms of smaller tense range and less explicit reference in pronouns, but also on other aspects (not directly analysed) such as vocabulary, length of np, for example, "the other man with the straight hair" versus "the man", and overall length of narrative. the c group's narratives reflected what could be described as "richness" as compared with the ε group's. this is illustrated in the following examples taken from an ε and c s's narratives:e s: "alright you see, the man brought this cow in and then they didn't know, they were pumping like water . . ." c s: "there was the three stooges and one of the big fat ones brings in the cow and then he says 'how do we milk the cow?' so another one of the three stooges takes the bottle and he puts it down and he says: 'give', and the cow is supposed to give milk and it doesn't so he goes to the back and he pulls its tail like you do with a water pump . . ." cognitive, linguistic and social factors can be considered in recounting for the differences in the communicative ability of the ε arid c groups: cognitive factors" ' two modes of style characteristic of the young child's egocentric language23 were identified in the ε group's narratives. these were juxtaposition and syncretism. for example ss tended to list events and did not link them in the causal, temporal and logical relationship conveyed in the task. according to bates,3 a particular level of cognitive maturity is required to transform information received into a meaningful the south african journal of communication disorders vol 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aspects of discourse in language impairment 13 message. since the ε ss were, according to their age, past the stage of 'egocentricity', some form of cognitive immaturity/delay may be suggested to account for their performance. linguistic factors in accounting for the results obtained, it is important to consider the characteristics of verbal discourse. firstly, it is produced in a situational context and not in isolation. this requires expression of semantic relations appropriate to the context. secondly, the linguistic formalisation of the semantic relations requires syntax which extends beyond the syntactic structure of a single sentence. it seems then that discourse places great demands on linguistic processing as well as cognitive capacity. it is possible that the ε group were unable to cope'on this level. social factors social factors, such as the ability to role-play, are important for effective communication. according to flavell," ellipsis and indefinite pronouns in a message are evidence of a basic failure on the part of the child to keep the listener's role in mind while communicating to him. this suggests that the ε group's increased use of ambiguous pronouns as compared with the c group may be related to their inability to role play. j in summary, it seems that the child's communicative ability in discourse should be viewed in the context of a combination of cognitive, linguistic and social factors. conclusion the present research has shown that a consideration of language beyond the single sentence level is relevant as it has highlighted differences between the ε and c groups. this implies the need for diagnosis and therapy of expressive language disorders in the older child to be considered with reference to discourse. failure to consider this may result in subtle language errors going undetected. it may also lead to the premature dismissal from therapy of children who demonstrate competence on the sentence level, but who have a more complex linguistic difficulty. it has been reported that children with a history of language disorders frequently have learning difficulties in later schooling2 — further highlighting the value of viewing language within a discourse framework. discourse appears to be a useful tool for assessing a broad range of language function as it taps semantic and syntactic aspects. specific discourse situations could be devised to evaluate and teach aspects important for effective communication. in therapy, the child's attention could be drawn to semantic relations as well as the appropriate expression of these relations. areas such as nonfluency, logical sequencing of events, appropriate use of pronouns in terms of indexing pronouns for reference and eliminating ambiguities, and abilities such as role-playing could be considered in a therapy program. situations such as having two or more actors of the same sex could be utilised to teach the use of pronouns. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 η. berger & α. sinoff it would seem that future research specifically directed to the application of discourse in diagnosis and therapy of the older language-impaired child would be of value. a ckno wledgements the writers would like to express their sincere thanks to ms c. penn, lecturer, dept. of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, for her guidance and help. references 1. antinucci, f., and miller, r. (1976): how children talk about what happened. 7. child lang., 3(2), 167-189. 2. arnold, g. (1969): patterns of predictive significance in preschool children with gross delay in speech and or language development. paper at the convention of the american speech and hearing association, chicago, illinois. 3. bates, e. (1976): language and context: the acquisition of pragmatics. academic press, new york. 4. beilin, h. (1975): studies in the cognitive basis of language development. academic press, new york. 5. bronckart, j., and sinclair, h. (1973): time, tense and aspect: cognition. int. j. cognitive psychol., 2, 107-131. 6. campbell, r., and wales, r. (1970): the study of language acquisition. in new horizons in linguistics, lyons, j. (ed.), penguin 242-260. 7. cazden, c.b. (1968): the acquisition of noun and verb inflections. child development, 39, 433-438. 8. cazden, c.b. (1972): the situation: a neglected source of social class differences in language use. in sociolinguistics. pride, j.b., and holmes, j., (eds.), penguin, 294-313. 9. chomsky, c. (1969): the acquisition of syntax in children from5to 10. research monograph no. 57 cambridge, massachusetts: the m.i.t. press. 10. cromer, r.f. (1970): "children are nice to understand": surface structure clues for the recovery of deep structure. brit. j. psychol., 61, 397-408. 11. flavell, j.h. (1968): the development of role taking and communication skills in children. john wiley and sons inc. n.y. 12. halliday, m.a.k. and hasan, r. (1976): cohesion in english. longman group ltd., london. , 13. haynes, w.o. and hood, s.b. (1978): dysfluency changes as a function of the systematic modification of linguistic complexity. j. commun. dis., 11, 79-93. 14. hendricks, w.o. (1973): essays on semiolinguistics and verbal art. mouton, the hague. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aspects of discourse in language impairment 15 15. herriot, p. (1969): the comprehension of tense by young children. child development, 40(1), 103-110. 16. huxley, r. (1970): the development of the correct use of subject personal pronouns in two children. in advances in psycholinguistics. flores d'arcais, g., and levelt, w., (eds.), w. amsterdam north hollard pub. co. 17. kessel, f.s. (1970): the role of syntax in children's comprehension from ages six to twelve. monographs of the society for research in child development, 35. 18. lee, l. (1974): developmental sentence analysis. northwestern university press, evanston. 19. lees, r.b. and klima, e.s. (1969): rules for english pronominalization. in modern studies in english. reibel, d.a. and schane, s.a. (eds.). prentice hall inc. new jersey. 20. limber, j. (1976): unravelling competence, performance and pragmatics in the speech of young children. j. child lang., 3, 309318. 21. menyuk, p. (1969): sentences children use. the m.i.t. press, cambridge. 22. palermo, d.s. and molfese, d.l. (1972): language acquisition from age five onward. psychological bulletin, 78(6), 409-428. 23. piaget, j. (1926): the language and thought of the child. routledge and kegan paul ltd., london. 24. pit corder, s. (1973): introducing applied linguistics. penguin. 25. quirk, r., and greenbaum, s. (1976): a university grammar of english. longman group ltd., london. 26. saling, m. (1977): lecturer, department of psychology, university of the witwatersrand. personal communication. 27. tuch, s. (1976): an aspect of "communicative competence" in older language impaired and normal children — their ability to produce coherent narrative sequences of sentences. unpublished research report, department speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. 28. van der geest, t„ gerstel, r., appel, r„ and tervoort, b. th. (1973): the child's communicative competence. mouton, the hague. 29. waryas, c.l. (1973): psycholinguistic research in language intervention programming: the pronoun system. j. psycholing. res., 2(3), 221-237. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) self contained 3track instrumentation tape recorder for scientific analysis igap four speed recorder: 1 5 , 7 ϊ ι 3 j e n d 1 ϊ i n / 8 · t w o d i r e c t recording t r a c k s a n d o n e f m t r a c k . applic a t i o n a s a precision sound level m e t e r w h e n a m e a s u r e m e n t microphone is c o n n e c t e d . specifications (typical values) unbalanced line input, impedance 100 kft. 10 mv for mpl (maximum peak level). line output at mpl, 1 v into 10 ki2 loudspeaker amplifier 1 w performance obtained by recording: frequency response, 20 db below mpl, 15-in/s 25 hz 35 khz * 1 , 0 d b ; 7 j i n / s 25 hz 20 khz * 1 , 0 db; 33 i n / s 25 hz 10 khz a 1,5 db; 1i i n / s 25 hz 3,5 khz a 1,5 db; at 1i i n / s recording from 2,5 hz and transposition for restitution. signal-to-noise ratio at 15 in/s, linear 57,db, asa a weighted ccir 64 db and nab 62 db distortion at mpl = 320 n w b / m . h 3 < 1,5% crosstalk attenuation at 10 khz a 50 db phase fluctuation a 12° between tracks at 7 i i n / s and 10 khz ^ fm-track at 15 and 7 j in/s: carrier frequency 17 khz maximum frequency deviation * 4 5 % peak-to-peak input voltage 2 v frequence response 0 hz to 4 khz 3 db signal-to-noise ratio 44 db speed stability at 15, 7 i and 33 in/s: * 0 . 1 % wow and flutter, din 45 507 weighted peak-to-peak value, 15 i n / s * 0 , 0 5 % , / 7 1 i n / s ±0,07%, 33,in/s 0,12%, 1i i n / s * 0 . 2 5 % , / ναβααπηιξηξξι vitaphone (pty) ltd v . p o b o x 4 6 3 7 , j o h a n n e s b u r g t e l : 3 7 1 4 5 4 / 5 5 t h f l o o r , c a p e y o r k h o u s e 2 5 2 j e p p e s t r e e t , j o h a n n e s b u r g t e l e x : 8 2 1 6 3 s a the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 15 assessment of speech intelligibility in five south-eastern bantu languages: critical ^ considerations marlene carno jacobson, ph.d (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg anthony traill, ph.d (witwatersrand) department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract this paper examines criteria underlying the development of tasks and materials for the measurement of speech intelligibility in five southeasternbantu languages. the chief considerations include utterance length, word familiarity and structure, and phonetic balance. it is established that the foundation research necessary for devising materials in south-eastern bantu languages on the same basis as those of fnelish has not yet been conducted. salient properties of the relevant african languages include multilingualism, dialectal variation, vocabulary differences between rural and urban speakers of the same language, borrowed words, the simple vowel systems, the fairly elaborate consonant systems prosodic features, certail syllable structure characteristics, and noun morphology. a rationale for the use of two measures of intelligibility is presented, while the need to adapt many criteria characterising english materials is demonstrated. opsomming . hierdie referaat ondersoek die kriteria wat onderliggend is aan die ontwikkeling van take en materiaal vir die meting van spraakverstaanbaarheid in vyf suid-oosterlike bantoe tale. die hoofoorwegings sluit lengte van uiting, woordbekendheid en -struktuur. en fonetiese batons in. dit is bevestigdat die basiese navorsing wat benodig is vir die saamstel van materiaal in suid-oostelike bantoe-tale nog nie op dieselfde basisas eneels eedoen is nie opvallende kenmerke van die relevante afrika-tale is veeltaligheid, dialektiese variasies, verskille in woordeskat tussen landelike en stedelike gebruikers van dieselfde taal, leenwoorde, die eenvoudige vokaalsisteme, die betreklik uitgebreide konsonantsisteme, nrosodiese kenmerke, sekere eienskappe van lettergreepstrukture, en naamwoordmorfologie. 'n rasionaal vir die gebruik van twee verstaanbaarheidsmetings is aangebied en die noodsaak om kenmerkende kriteria vir engelse materiaal aan te pas, word ook uitgewys. the present paper explores the range of issues confronted on undertaking research which sought to measure the speech intelligibility levels of a series of surgically-treated oral cancer patients each of whom spoke one of five south-eastern bantu languages (viz. jacobson, 1986). the problem was approached by critically "dissecting" the notion of intelligibility in an effort to extract salient properties and, thereafter, by extrapolating applicable criteria of intelligibility measures to the set of phonetico-linguistic conditions characterising these languages. as such, the major concerns underlying speech intelligibility in the present paper are two-fold. the reconciliation of these two aspects forms the rationale for the choice of designated measures of intelligibility, and is represented in the materials themselves. i nine members of the south-eastern bantu family of african languages are recognised and have developed as educational media and literary languages in south africa (south africa, 1985): | 1. nguni languages: zulu, xhosa, swazi, and ndebele, among a total of twelve nguni languages. 2. sotho languages: southern sotho, northern sotho and tswana, among a total of eleven sotho languages. 3. tsonga. 4. venda. the present paper happens to deal with the nguni and sotho groups of indigenous languages, each of which is sub-divided to form dialect clusters. it was coincidental that the languages spoken by the randomly selected subjects of the study reflect the five most frequently spoken bantu languages in south africa, namely, zulu, xhosa, northern sotho, southern sotho, and tswana. in view of the number of dialects, some of which are poorly known, certain dialects have been raised to standard forms by taking into account various factors, such as the prominence of a die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 leader, or tribe size (ziervogel, louw, ferreira, baumbach & lombard, 1967). a second major feature of this group of languages is the profound multilingual situation which characterises speakers of the bantu languages of south africa. no materials appear to have been published previously in the relevant african languages for the specific purpose of measuring intelligibility. speech discrimination word lists used in audiology (e.g. baragwanath hospital, 1977) and phonetic inventories (e.g. hillbrow hospital, 1981) did not meet those requirements deemed fundamental to the study concerned, namely, clear evidence of the formulation of a set of criteria for item selection, taking into account previous research on intelligibility. speech intelligibility is the property of speech communication involving meaning, and is the quality or state of being comprehended or understood. a signal is intelligible to a listener, and for a speech signal to have intelligibility, both a speaker and a listener must be involved (lehiste & peterson, 1959). hence, because both speech production and speech perception are involved, distinctly more complexity is implied than is suggested by the use of superficially related terminology, such as articulation, recognisability, identifiability, and discriminability, which do not necessarily pertain to speech communication, consider meaning, or take full account of the listener. numerous factors determine this "communicative effectiveness". in the main, these are the social context, message content, the stimulus signal and the medium used for signal transmission, the speaker and his speech mechanism, and the characteristics of the listener. speech intelligibility is employed by numerous disciplines (e.g. speech pathology, audiology, and acoustical engineering), each of which places differential emphasis on these major elements of the communication process. in the research on which this paper is based, the emphasis was on speaker characteristics with an attempt being made to control all other parameters as closely as possible, so that non-speaker parameters could serve © sasha 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 • ii thic naner seeks to determine l a r g e l y as constants^ es^ntially^ thus ̂ ^ ^ intelligibility in a optimal message content in oraer valid fashion. . λΐηο feature of speech inteuigibility is its performance ^ ^ ^ υ 0 * γ a „ d " y m a n 1981). as such, it supersedes several related criteria and models' which capture a more limited scope. these include qualitative, perceptually-based tools used for differential diagnosis (e.g. darley, aronson & brown, 1975), the evaluation of individual components of speech production, such as defective articulation (e.g. prather, 1960), phonation, or resonance, and models which systematically evaluate the anatomical or physiological locus of disturbance along the vocal tract, such as the "point-place system" of rosenbek and lapointe (1978). the inclusive nature of the concept of intelligibility is further exemplified by its pertinence to different speech-disordered groups (e.g. the cerebral palsied, alaryngeal speakers, cases of congenital orofacial malformations, among numerous others), despite the varying nature of the pathological elements of the speech production process. the concept of intelligibility is therefore sufficiently flexible to house a range of speech production variables, varying in combination and degree. it is thereby possible for each clinical population to bring to research its associated symptomatological milieu and still be investigated under the broad umbrella of intelligibility. the potential information offered by intelligibility data is diverse, depending on both task selection and on the level of analysis undertaken: the more detailed the analysis, the richer and more versatile the applications of the data. in the literature, the analysis and presentation of intelligibility data assume a variety of forms: 1. simple percentages of intelligibility reflecting the number of discrete units perceived correctly. 2. analysis of perception-production confusions to form matrices (e.g. nichols, 1976). 3. analysis of the distance between produced and perceived phonemes (in words) in terms of distinctive features: manner, place or voicing differences. 4. breakdown into analysis of vowel and consonant intelligibility, once the basic word intelligibility scores have been computed. 5. the determination of the patholect for a particular clinical population, or the pattern of phonemic confusions for a particular clinical group. 6. combination of inteuigibility measures with other measures such as speech rate, for example "intelligible words per minute" (iwpm) and "unintelligible words per minute" (uwpm) — yorkston and beukelman (1981). 7. description of auditory or perceptual characteristics of the speech. 8. development of a classification system for recognition of different groups demonstrating a particular speech disorder. 9. development of an intelligibility measure which reflects severity of involvement such as shriberg and kwiatkowski's (1982) percentage consonants correct (pcc), which is, however, defined as "a measure of articulatory proficiency". methods of assessment of surgically-treated oral cancer patients apparently sample the range of procedures generally employed in the examination of intelligibility in english-speaking pathological populations. the range of methods described in current and older research is extensive. however, whereas older research glosses informally through accounts of intelligibility, current research appears to aim at greater qualitative and quantitative specificity, marlene carno jacobson and anthony traill with certain intelligibility materials even being designed for particular clinical populations and the ranges of severity within them. standardised word lists frequently constitute the materials of speech intelligibility assessment, e.g. the cid w.22 phonetically balanced (pb) word lists (hirsh, davis, silverman, reynolds, eldert & benson, 1952), as employed in numerous studies (e.g. skelly, 1973); the consonant-syllable nucleus-consonant words (lehiste and peterson, 1959), as selected and adapted by tikofsky (1970), for example, for use with dysarthrics; the fairbanks (1958) test of phonemic differentiation (a "rhyme" test), as used by kipfmueller and prins (1971), for example; the word lists of black (1957), as used by yorkston and beukelman (1981), for example; and moses' (1969) monosyllabic word lists (as cited by nichols, 1976). standardised sentence materials (e.g. the cid "everyday sentences") have also been utilised in examining the construct and criterion validity of various intelligibility procedures (schiavetti, metz and sitler, 1981). finally, standardised phonetically balanced passages of reading, such as "my grandfather", "the rainbow passage" and "arthur the rat", (fairbanks, 1960), have been employed as elicitation material for intelligibility measurement (e.g. piatt, andrews, young and neilson, 1978). however, the use of read material introduces a prerequisite of patient literacy, which was inappropriate in the research concerned. a further batch of materials includes those designed for specific clinical populations, such as the glossal and labial word lists for surgically-treated oral cancer patients (e.g. skelly, 1973). in contrast, other research methods are individualistic from the viewpoint of the researcher, for example, the use of read sentences from early reading books in the case of deaf school-leavers (kyle, conrad, mckenzie, morris & weiskrantz, 1978), picture description (markides, 1978), and spontaneous speech samples (weiss, 1978, cited by schmidt, 1984). under the latter circumstances, the properties of the spoken material are clearly far less stringently controlled. as mentioned above, no similar spectrum of formalised materials exists in the languages under consideration. nonetheless, within the above range, certain themes and issues pertaining to the choice and characteristics of materials recur with remarkable regularity, thereby illustrating the origins of useful criteria for devising appropriate intelligibility measures. these may be categorised in terms of three key components: i a. the composition of speech materials, which takes into account the message to be communicated, its length, complexity,! and spontaneity. i b. the form of response required of the listener, in order that he may register his reaction to or understanding of the message, either by written or spoken means. c. the choice of general procedural characteristics influencing the reliability and validity of the assessment, such as the manner in which stimuli are presented, and the selection of judges. the first component, the composition of spoken materials, is the chief area of interest in the present paper. the second component extends beyond the focus selected for the present discussion. only isolated aspects of the third component are relevant here: irrespective of the nature of the materials that are selected, or the manner in which the listener responds to the signal, the mass of methodological details pertaining to stimulus presentation and listener characteristics is largely independent of the language of the speaker and thus warrants routine consideration. such factors the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a s s e s s m e n t of speech intelligibility in five south-eastern bantu languages: critical considerations 17 include the decision to pre-record the speech stimuli or to present hese live whether to use the same stimuli repeatedly, the selection of suitable carrier phrases, and details of judge selection and training. certain aspects of judge selection are, however, incorporated in the discussion below. furthermore, this review proceeds from overriding linguistic properties of the five languages, largely of a general, sociolinguistic nature — with implications at a semantic level, and progresses towards the enumeration of more specific, phonetic, phonological and morphological characteristics. all of these considerations affect the choice of individual word items. key considerations in devising appropriate materials a. composition of speech materials 1. utterance length while single words regularly form the basis of speech intelligibility scores, researchers frequently comment that singleword performance does not permit the evaluation of several critical parameters of speech, such as the effects of juncture, rhythm and stress patterns, and rate control (e.g. griggs, 1958). furthermore, the addition of context requires less fine discriminations of the listener (miller, heise & lichten, 1951). as such, there appears to be a demand for the development of formal materials suitable for studying the intelligibility of connected speech and its intrinsic prosodic properties. towards meeting this need, schiavetti, sitler, metz and houde (1983) recently established two formulae for calculating contextual speech intelligibility on the basis of single-word intelligibility scores. in spite of the above-mentioned criticism, nichols (1976) reports that excellent correlations exist between pb scores and sentence intelligibility scores for esophageal speakers: " . . . hence, word intelligibility measures may be used as estimates of connected speech abilities with confidence". hodson and paden (1982) cite research with phonologically-impaired children demonstrating that essentially the same processes were revealed by single-word elicitation procedures as by both delayed sentence imitations, and by connected speech samples. this work therefore plays down, to some extent, the presumed effects of utterance length. yorkston and beukelman (1978) support the above findings noting that mean intelligibility scores for sentences were not different from scores derived from single-word tests. however, for transcription tasks, there was an interaction between severity and intelligibility score's on sentences versus words, that is, the most intelligible speakers tended to score higher jon sentence transcriptions, while the least intelligible speakers received higher scores on single words. the same researchers also found that various measurement techniques, including bojth sentences and single words, tended to rank speakers similarly. beukelman and yorkston (1979) confirmed the close relationship between both single-word and sentence intelligibility, and the amount of information transferred between a speaker and a listener. interestingly enough, piatt, andrews, young an/ iii i 111*1 11 hi 1*111 /1//8//,]*///11. vowels /a/ it i /e/ m h! lol /u/ voiceless dental aspirated dental nasalised dental delayed voice dental fully voiced denta] preceding naaal+volceless voiceless alveopalatal aspirated alveopalatal nasaliaed alveopalatal delayed voice alveopalatal fully voiced alveopalatal preceding nasal+voiceless voiceless lateral aspirated lateral delayed voice lateral nasalised lateral fully voiced lateral preceding nasal+voiceless glottals ihl voiceless fricative voiced fricative table 1 contains the phonemic inventories of zulu, xhosa, northern sotho, southern sotho and tswana, based on the works of brown (1971), cole (1955), doke (1931), doke and mofokeng (1957), jordan (1966), khumalo (1981), mabille and dieterlen (1961), ziervogel et al. (1967), ziervogel, louw and taljaard (1981), and ziervogel and mokgokong (1975). these inventories are presented in order to identify the phonemic constituents of the south-eastern bantu languages and to informally observe differences from english. to capture subtle phonetic differences between ostensibly similar articulations belonging to different languages (e.g. / b / in zulu as compared to the sotho languages), finer phonetic specifications are incorporated within the phonemic description (viz. / b / versus / b / : murmured versus voiced / b / ) . the purpose of depicting certain phonetic variants, comprising members of consonant clusters or nasal compounds; is that clusters are particularly complex and therefore most taxing phonetically — quite conceivably affecting intelligibility. for this reason they would add an additional dimension of complexity to intelligibility word lists. in table 1 elements of clusters or nasal compounds are marked with a "c". 1 dialectal variants are incorporated in table 1 too, primarily to mark alternative correct productions which could be misconstrued as errors, and for the sake of completeness. these are marked with a "d". in conducting a single-word intelligibility task, dialectal variation would need to be taken into account and should not incur penalty. for example, in northern sotho, the labio-alveolar fricative, /#e/ , may also be produced as /te/ , or / h / , or / f / )ziervogel et al. 1967). certain segments appear only within ideophones in the canonical forms cited above. an ideophone is an onomatopoeic word form with its own morphology, phonology and prosodic rules (kunene, 1978). in view of these properties, the controversial definition of ideophones (wilkes, 1978), and their frequent appearance within a particular grammatical structure, such as "ho re . . ." (southern sotho), from which it may be indivisible, it would seem unwise to consider ideophones as word items. this view would hold irrespective of whether an ideophone contained a required phoneme. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a s e s s m e n t o f speech intelligibility in five south-eastern bantu languages: critical considerations 21 tensive range of consonants in each of the above languages ared to english, has implications for the length of word lists c ° uwould be desirable to include all consonants more than once for reliability purposes. m d a r i s o n of the phonemes of zulu, xhosa, southern sotho, u r t h e r n sotho and tswana, reveals language-specific trends rtable 1)· the vast repertoire of clicks, voiced and voiceless f lives λ / and / y , and murmured forms o f / b / , / d / , and l are characteristic of zulu and xhosa. xhosa also has an extended range of palatal phonemes, depicted orthographically as " t y " and "dy", and the ejected velar affricate a w . in addition any zulu fricatives are pronounced as affricates in xhosa, for example, "inhloko" is "intloko", and "insimu" is pronounced "intsimi". particular to the sotho group of languages are the alveolar trill, / r / the aspirated lateral stop, / t l h / , the voiced alveopalatal fricative, μ , and the labio-palatal "double" phonemes (affricate-like, generally characterised by two virtually simultaneous, non-adjacent places of articulation), /f s /, / , /pw, and / w · members of the latter group tend to be dialectal in tswana. characteristic of northern sotho are the labio-alveolar phonemes, /to/, ,/ps·/ and fysw tswana contains marked dialectal variation, which influences the production of alveolar and alveopalatal affricates, and also leads to the non-lingual voiceless fricative also being produced as either / ( / or / h / . all five languages display phonemic aspiration, ejection of consonants, and the palatal nasal phoneme, μ . as a group these therefore show a variety of airstream mechanisms in speech: glottalic ingressive and egressive, velaric, as well as pulmonic airstream mechanisms. where vowel phonemes are concerned, south-eastern bantu languages have five to seven pure vowels as a rule, no central vowels, and no diphthongs. the nguni languages, xhosa and zulu, both have five basic vowel phonemes with one variant each of / e / and / o / , acting in complementary distribution as a result of adjacent vowel influence. the sotho languages, southern sotho, northern sotho and tswana, have seven basic vowels and four additional phonetic variants (ziervogel et al. 1967). b. response formats j the formalised materials discussed above require that the listener's response consists of' repetition, transcription, written completion of a word with one letter, or completion of multiplechoice response formats. it would appear though that transcription or stimulus repetition serves as the closest means of reflecting the listener's actual perceptions. although multiplechoice formats have excellent quantifiability and recognised usefulness, analysis of such tasks indicates that these may well test identifiability rather than intelligibility. the listener's "perceptions" are structured to fit a particular framework and real perceptions may be ignored. furthermore it has been demonstrated that multiple-choice response formats may be graded in difficulty by systematically altering the information provided to the judges, while keeping the stimulus constant (yorkston & beukelman, 1980). in this instance there is an inverse relationship between the number of selection options and inteuigibility (ibid.). the influential effects of various response formats with specified options suggest that these may contaminate the results of intelligibility tests and are best avoided. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 where test materials comprise connected speech, rating scales are commonly used to capture gradations of speech intelligibility. these clearly demand a different type of subjectivity to that of the procedures described previously, in that they rely on appraisals rather than the listener's actual comprehension of speech. however, the appropriateness of different rating scales deserves judicious consideration to ensure the selection of valid indices of speech intelligibility (cf. schiavetti, metz & sitler, 1981). many of the issues concerning response formats are examined by yorkston and beukelman (1978). these researchers conclude that it may be beneficial to take advantage of the task hierarchy that apparently exists among quantification procedures, instead of relying on a single measurement technique to quantify speech across the entire performance range. however, this suggestion would appear more suited to clinical practice than research, as in the case of the latter, a standard methodology is necessarily implemented. the nature of redundancy the foregoing discussion reveals various factors which could increase the redundancy of the speech signal during intelligibility testing. these include the offer of options in identifying stimulus words, the use of linguistic context, the exclusive selection of singular verb imperatives, and an increase in the number of syllables per stimulus word (certainly for english). however, redundancy in a word inteuigibility task is an undesirable feature in that it meddles with the attempt to measure "absolute" inteuigibility. meyerson, johnson and weitzman(1980)comment on the contribution of redundancy to the intelligibility glossectomees, reporting a 50 per cent discrepancy in intelligibility scores based on single words versus connected speech. in the present south-eastern bantu languages, there is likely to be some degree of redundancy in single words existing independently of that contained in connected speech, largely due to the multisyllabic, tonal nature of words in these languages. as such redundancy is potentially introduced at the word level in the form of semantic and grammatical tonal cues. these properties, in conjunction with fairly consistent syllable shapes and sequences, conceivably provide supplementary cues to single-word intelligibility stimuli. on a morphological level, furthermore, many single words in the bantu languages are polymorphic, with all prefixes and some suffixes showing grammatical tone. in this regard, it is noteworthy that in zulu, for example, noun prefixes are composed of only three vowels and seven consonants (westphal, 1973). this limited range could possibly aid the listener's decision when the noun stem is doubtful. on the other hand, noun stems are so varied that it is uncertain whether the class prefix effectively contributes any redundancy to the speech signal, in spite of its tonal characteristics and phonetic composition. it would seem, nonetheless, that if grammatical tone does add redundancy to work items, this would operate in the case of nouns predominantly. in attempting to eliminate all the above-mentioned potential sources of redundancy in a word inteuigibility task, the syllable composition of the words would probably need to be restricted to being as short but as representative as possible, the noun class prefix would have to be limited to only one syllable, and the use of options in the response format would be rejected. conclusion a detailed account has been presented of paramount considerations in resolving the issue of task and material selection r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 marlene carno jacobson and anthony traill for the measurement of intelligibility in five south-eastern ban u languages. the final outcome of the effort to attain maximally vthd measures was therefore arrived at under the following restrictive circumstances: 1. t h e a b s e n c e o f a d e w e y t y p e i n d e x o f r e l a t i v e p h o n e m e frequency; . 2. the absence of a thorndike-lorge-type index of word frequency for languages other than southern sotho; 3. the absence of standardised passages of reading; 4. the paucity of intelligibility materials for english-speaking glossectomees to act as guidelines for the development of materials in african languages. the decision was made to couple two complementary tasks: a single-word task, which may be viewed as a "molecular-intensive" or detailed analytical measure, while rating of a spontaneous speech task comprises a "molar-extensive" or global measure (hollenbeck, 1978), and may be regarded as a "molar taxonomy", as it combines a "a number of features, actions, directions, and objects of behaviour" (sackett, 1978: 25). this decision was determined by three factors which receive elaboration below: the specific research objectives, the emergency of two distinctive genres of intelligibility measures, and the limitations of any single measure of intelligibility in isolation. firstly, in considering the purpose of the research, there was dual interest in "how well speech was understood", which indexes overall speech effectiveness, as well as in specific error patterns. it seems logical that there be an underlying commonality to both processes and one would predict that the number of errors relates closely to the understandability of contextual speech. however, two discrete tasks were deemed necessary: it could not be assumed that the percentage word intelligibility based on unstandardised materials would accurately parallel the level of spontaneous speech intelligibility; neither could it be assumed that a word list, assembled on the basis of strict criteria such as phoneme position and syllable structure, would reflect the properties of everyday speech and vocabulary. the use of spontaneous speech samples was therefore important for the criterion validation of an unstandardised work intelligibility task. hence, a word intelligibility task incorporating the phoneticophonological properties of the relevant language was desirable in that 1. phonemes could be included in a controlled and structured manner; 2. the lack of context normally provided by syntax would allow evaluation of each phoneme in a minimally redundant form; 3. a numerically specific index of the percentage of intelligible words could be computed; 4. words are the work-horse of intelligibility materials in english, and serve as highly accessible units of speech for a researcher who is not a fluent speaker of a foreign language. similarly, a spontaneous speech task was desirable in that 1. ratings of performance on such a task could tackle the construct of speech intelligibility directly as everyday connected speech would be typified, providing a reasonable approximation of the normal communication process; 2. the task promotes the linguistic style and vocabulary of the subject and thereby captures a congruent communicative whole. isolated words, in contrast, are in danger of falling peripheral to either the judge's or the subject's vocabulary, despite careful item selection. the particular method whereby spontaneous speech samples may be evaluated, constitutes a separate methodological decision which lies outside the scope of the present paper. the lists of single words in zulu, xhosa, northern sotho, southern sotho and tswana, appear in appendixes 1-5. as far as possible, these were formulated and items selected according to the following criteria: 1. vocabulary items should be familiar to both rural and urban speakers of the relevant language. 2. the word structure should be highly representative of the particular language in question. a scan of common vocabulary items in nguni and sotho languages, combined with the notion of shortest word having least redundancy, suggested that it was most appropriate to include bisyllables in the sotho languages where the simplest uninfected nouns and verbs have a cvcv (nasal) structure, and trisyllables in the nguni languages where nouns of a vcvcv structure predominate because of the pre-prefix. although cvcv verb imperatives are readily available in the nguni languages, the final v is always / a / which acts to increase the redundancy of stimuli in that the final vowel is always given. 3. all consonant phonemes in each language should be included twice in the list, once as the first consonant in a word and secondly, in an intervocalic position. 4. within the scope of these criteria, the number of items presented to the judges was restricted to as few as possible by eliminating the least satisfactory items. this represented an attempt to increase both reliability and validity. the least satisfactory items in the research were those of doubtful conversational familiarity. it was hoped that the remaining items would constitute sufficiently long tests and that these would be more reliable, comprehensive and valid than lists that would have been any shorter. 5. rigorous balancing of vowel phonemes was not undertaken due to the stringency of primary requirements already mentioned. an attempt was made, however, to keep the frequency of different vowel phonemes equivalent within each word list. secondly, the above exposition of intelligibility and related issues reveals two distinguishable categories of assessment methods: the first category, comprising formalised procedures, displays a certain stringency of both item selection (the maximal units of which are sentences), and procedural design (which facilitates quantification and error analysis). however, english procedures of this nature are at times of questionable validity, in terms of the definition of intelligibility provided earlier in this paper, as these could be more accurately regarded as exercises in stimulus discriminability. in the case of south-eastern bantu languages, materials of this nature could be formulated through the informal application of principles such as word familiarity, word length, and phonetic balance. | ι in contrast, less formalised procedures, in that either the material or the assessment procedure itself is irregular, perhaps represent more valid appraisals of intelligibility. these procedures generally have, as a minimal unit, the sentence, and usually consist of either read or spoken samples of connected speech. hence, these demonstrate high face validity in representing overall speech behaviour, which is of prime interest. however, in this instance, the validity of the listener's task or tool of judgment may be questionable in relation to the construct of speech intelligibility. / these observations suggest that both formal and less formal procedures have merits and demerits, and probably represent complementary entities, which are most effective in unison. thirdly, as demonstrated by english materials, although the above range of intelligibility measures and procedures is vast, in most cases, only a portion of overall speech performance is r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a s s e s s m e n t of speech intelligibility in five south-eastern bantu languages: critical considerations 23 c a p t u r e d by a single measure, which furthermore, cannot be employed to describe performance across the entire severity range: as all measures have both a ceiling and a floor effect, these are sensitive to performance changes in only a limited range. these limitations underscore the strength of any single measure of intelligibility in research which is combined with at least one other intelligibility task, or the event of intelligibility comprising only one measure within a battery of measures of speech production, where such a battery also incorporates acoustic, articulatory, and physiological studies. additional measures then serve as concurrent validation procedures. if "the observer's perception and subsequent judgment represents the final validity for the identification and measurement of disordered speech" (young and downs, 1968: 6), the present paper comprises a preliminary step in the direction of reflecting this criterion, that is, intelligibility, among speakers of southeastern bantu languages. a ckno wled g ε me nts the human sciences research council, pretoria, and the senior bursary fund, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, are gratefully acknowledged for their generous financial support of the doctoral research on which this paper is based. inhlanzi igazi ixhegu iqanda umeqo izinyo insimu uqhotho iviki iphepha iconsi umpompi igeja umgqomo iwashi indlebe idlelo induku umshado inantshi ilanga inzule i jaha amakha ingiqi isiklabhu igceke ingqondo uxamu phonemic transcription /inti andzi/ /igazl/ /i//hcgu/ /i!anda/ /uml!d/ /iziro/ /ints imu/ λι! hot\v /ufudu/ /ivik'i/ /iph'pha/ /i/onts 'i/ /ump -̂ mp i/ /ig'd ψ/ /izi.e»v /iliklo/ /induk u/ /um fad o/ /inant j"'i/ /ila^a/ /indzule/ /id^aha/ /amakha/ / i rji ι i / /isiki 'abu/ /i/gek'r./ /irj'.gondo/ /u//amu/ gloss fish blood old man an egg a jump field shrivelled thing; homeless person tortoiae week paper drop of liquid a tap a plough sheep barrel; drum; "round person" goat a vatch pasture a stick a wedding naartjie fruit sun; day type of indigenous cow strapping fellow perfume, scent lazy person sheep courtyard mind; brain leguaan appendices igxathu /i//gathu/ stride (n) ingxangxa / if)//gar)//ga/ a green-striped frog appendix 1 : zulu word list igxolo /i//§.ilo/ bark of tree orthography phonemic gloss icici /i/i/i/ earring transcription. ipapa /ip 'ap 'a/ porridge idada /idada/ duck umunwe /umunwe/ a finger ungcede / /uo/gede/ common little bird appendix 2 : xhosa word list unyawo /upawo/ leg orthography phonemic gloss inxele /in//eie/ left-handed person transcription isifo /isifo/ sickness inzala /indzala/ progeny; interest (on capital) itiye /it 'ΐτε/ tea indlovu /indtpvu/ elephant incwadi /in/wadi/ a book indyebo /inre co/ treasure; plenty inyoni /ipdni/ bird ityhefu /ic h,-fu/ poison (n) umhambi /um ̂ambi/ traveller intyelo /inc'rio/ information ugogo /ueogo / grandmother intlahla /inti'aia/ shiny, glossy appearance ithanga /ithana/ pumpkin ibhokwe /ibokw 't·/ goat ingxoxo a conversation irafu /ixafu/ tax, hut-tax, poll tax iqhude /i!hudt/ a cock igronya /iiopa/ sack-cloth, coarse cloth; sack ihhashi / i fia j i / horse ikrele /ikx 'εΐε/ stabbing assegai, sword, sickle indlovu /indlovu/ elephant ihashe /ihaje/ horse, mount; abcess imvubu /im»vubu/ hippo imvubu /im<4vu£u/ hippopotamus inhlanhla /int* 'anti a/ luck indoda /indoda/ man, husband /uzip o/ intsimi /ints 'imi/ arable field, garden uzipho /uzip o/ nail i ihlombe /iiambe/ shoulder imfene /im)wi!i/ igatya /igac 'a/ umhambi /umtambi/ iqands /i!anda/ ichibi /i/hifci/ igxalaba /i//gala&a/ inxele /in//clc/ umnqwazi /umn!wazi/ ingwanqwa /i η!w an!a/ icangci /i/arygi/ inkcazo /ink/azy uncipho /un/ip h y inkxvaleko /ink//walek 'ο / umngqusho /umqkiu/o/ ingqumbo / i>)! gun bo / injini /ind^ini/ ijaji / itĵ ad̂ i/ umgcini /um/gini/ umgodi /urngô i/ ingoti /irjgozi/ inkqu /irjklu/ unoqho /uno1ho/ iaixa /isi//a/ ungwebi /umgwcm/ appendix 3 : northern sotho word list taebe /te' ebe/ pudi /p 'udi/ dijo /di^o/ gloss something circular; earning; small group . time, period; watch, clock . door (not a doorway); cell in a honeycomb • conversation old man • doctor, medicine-man, witch-doctor bag, pouch, sack, pocket pupil, scholar, reader, disciple, student • loaves of bread • paper; page • feeling, senaation; view, opinion; impression • root • button, buckle; coin; seashell discovery rat councillor; splinter jar a roast • joy· gladness, rejoicing watch, clock summer change (n) arrow dam, reservoir bee mane leeway granary a gulp, swallow clause; branch traveller egg; large bead pool, pond, lake shoulder, shoulder-blade left hand; left-handed person cap, hat step piece of corrugated iron; metal sheet memorandum; explanation decline, decrease misery eamp anger engine judge one who keeps deep excavation, mine; a hole danger; misfortune, accident, injury reality clothes-peg bunch; handful (of grasa, corn flowers) those who judge • ear orthography noka kgogo topa toro bjala botse ngaka nyala tsea hlogo tlala khuta phiri motho p6hap6ha tshipi tlhaba psinya holla dinku ngwana motse nnosi tehwene sego kofi hwile fase gohle legong tshwane leho ' swiri maswi pholo ngwaga robja monna haka jase patsi kgomo 11a nnyane mpheng gomme thunya kua sebopsa ngwe lefaega ipehina phonemic transcription /nok 'a/ /kx 'οϊο/ /t ops/ /toro/ /p3ala/ /bots e/ /oak 'a/ /moo/ /psla/ /tj'ea/ /ioio/ / tl 'ala/ /khut 'a/ /phiri/ /motho/ /pjhapjha/ /tj'ipi/ /tlhaba/ /ps 'ipa/ /foia/ /dii'jk 'u / /^wana/ /mots e/ /aoji/ /tj 'wene/ /i+iele/ /ap'a/ /sets/ /k'ofi/ /hwile/ /fase/ /roie/ /le*ov /tshwane/ /lets/ /«fcsiri/ /maai/ /pholo/ /twa^a/ /rop̂ a/ /morla/ /f-aka/ /d^aae/ /pat f i/ /wet j 'we/ /kx 'omo/ /la/ /p/hatl'a/ /pane/ /mp he /xome/ /thupa/ /kua/ /sebop j 'a/ /me/ /na/ /rjwe/ /lefje*a/ /ipjhina/ river chicken pick up dream; prickly pear plant; beer beautiful doctor maater, owner; one, other (adj.) to marry, give dowry take, receive, get hold, take on; marry head hunger hide hyena, wolf person applaud, clap iron; bell slaughter, to stab a person defaeeate! child village, town baboon body stomach; skipping rope; sky between two clouds calabash coffee has passed away; dead under, below, down everywhere wood, timber pretoria spoon, ladle lemon ox year break, break off (paaaive of "roba") man · to hook on overcoat i coat i fall toward; to fail in (causative of "wela") ! heard of cattle ^ cry; mourn; ring; strike (a bell) play a musical instrument; bellow; yelp; mew; whistle to break (into pieces); break in (to burgle); smash small, little, miniature a handle and; furthermore cauae duat; blossom; ache; explode -·over there creature my / our mother i, me another faacea of diarrhoea; a coward enjoy, be happy he south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a s s e s s m e n t of speech intelligibility in five south-eastern bantu languages: critical considerations 25 phonemic transcription /mokx'wa/ /dirapfa/ . squtherh flotho l i s t phonemic ianscripti01 /pita'a/ /buk'a/ /thipha/ /noma/ /leino/ /k 'olol/ /kx 'aut'a/ /hemp e/ /ict'o/ /tahela/ /iitsha/ /nt j"'a/ /pjhatl'a/ /net 'e/ /moho/ /lei \ra/ /kx'aba/ /phiri/ /mat he/ /φ» ana/ /di^>/ /me/ /dutl 'a/ /diha/ /foia/ /haok'a/ /tl'atl'a/ /tlhok'a/ /j wa]a/ /ta'oie/ /tshwtpha/ /mots o/ /!a!a/ /mona/ /tj 'otj'a/ /tjnlo/ /motjhi/ /kx'wa*a/ /^ak'a/ /sits'a/ /madi/ /pjhana/ /pj 'atl'a/ /t'oaa/ /b^arats a/ /porwa/ /la/ /pjhehs/ /ab^wa/ /md-kx'0/ /iata 'wa/ lefjwa /lefjwa/ die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif mokgwa dimpsa pitsa buka thipa nana leino koloi kgauta hempe qeto tahela ntsha ntja pjhatla nnete mmoho leqhwa kgaba phiri ma the ngwana dijo eheshe dutla diha fohla hanka tlatla tlhoka shwaya tsohle tshwepha motsho qaqa monna tjotja tjhello motjhi kgwahla jaka sitsa madi pjhanya pjatla tosa bjaratsa nyorwa 11a pjheha abjwa mookgo hlatswa • manner, custom, habit • dogs gloss pot, vessel book knife meat tooth wagon, car gold; a big gold or silver bead shirt the end, conclusion; decision to cross (a road); to cut meat; to live; to pour to take out, to aend, tp eontribute dog; despised person; dysentry of children; very heavy stone to spesk much, to tell; to break into pieces truth together; on one side ice to be besutiful; to dress well; a spoon; to have a nice action hyena saliva child, infant; tooth of a baby; charm used for frightening birds away food; crops violence; type of tree to leak, trickle out, drip to cause to fall; to throw, to cast down, to put into trouble to peel; to enlarge s hole to walk proudly to shine; a wooden dish (for meat) hardened ear-wax; to shout 'to prick, to sting, cause a rash all, everything to twist, entwine black to be plain, evident r man, husband jto continue raining jdamage sustained through fire not anywhere to run; to become atrong, firm; to shrivel up to go and live in a foreign place to give, to grant, to favour with blood; beer to speak, to say to boil well to stretch out, to raise to crunch, to smash to become thirsty weep, cry, emit a sound, bellow to have diarrhoea, to be purged; to act in a cowardly manner to divide, hand out, distribute tears (n) to wash vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 orthography nku kgafa nna mme tshwara kgomo yaka repha appendix 5 mantswe mollo kgopa tlase podi taie tshimo thuto ruri koko moagi ngaka nyals rwele jala tshups lwana tshweu tshwara gagwe nkgvana dinku yaalo mow a yona leswe puo tau kobo sejo nama rre gotlhe pitsi tshipi khudu phutha tlhaba kgopho phepha fofs wesi tefo senya rragwe rrunidi phonemic transcription /ik'u/ /kx'afa/ /na/ /m/ /tshwara/ /kx omo/ /jak'a/ /rt-pha/ nnye nnetlane nkga tlhwaya (tsebe) 11a : tswana word list /msnta we/ /molo/ /kx'op'a/ /tl'ase/ /p'odi/ /ts'ie/ /tsh imo/ /thut'o/ /ruri/ /k'ok'o/ /moaxi/ /oak'a/ /pala/ /rwele/ /d^ala/ /tsnjps/ /lwana/ /ts weu/ /tshwara/ /gagwe/ /t}kx 'wons/ /dirjku/ /jsalo/ /mowa/ /jona/ /leswe/ /puo/ /t'au/ /kobo/ /sed^o/ /nama/ /rt/ /x3tlhe/ /p'its'i/ /tship'i/ /khudu/ /•ε/ /phutha/ /tlhaba/ /kx'opho/ /phepha/ /fofs/ /wesi/ /t'efo/ /sepa/ /raxwe/ /midi/ /n·/ /netl'ane/ /nkx1 a/ /tlhhwajs/ /la/ gloss sheep • tax, hut-tax-duties • i, myself; to be; continually mother of; and, also, indeed to seize, to get hold of, to capture a herd of cattle it seemed that to become loose, old • words • fire snail; to trip up, stumble, offend, vex, displease down a gost locust a garden lesson, education true grandmother a builder a doctor to marry hss worn plant:· wesvil fight, quarrel, strive with each other white hold his, hers a water pot, calabash sheep like that air it dirty, nasty apeech lion a kaross; blanket food meat father, my fsther entirely, all a horse; a zebra iron, metal tortoise my mother to gather, collect to slaughter, stab β small bush with edible fruit clean fly! own; alone payment spoil) his father mealie little something to emit s smell to listen carefully cry r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 orthography phonemic transcription gloss lwantsha /lwantsha/ cause to quarrel or strive with each other lotswa /lot] 'ws/ be required, demanded, asked foi „pho /mpho/ gift sekhi /sekhi/ a species of thorny bush supa /supa/ to show, point; seven aeba /stba/ to whisper, to backbite aga /axa/ to build; live, dwell fitlha /fitlha/ to arrive; to bury, hide kgakge /kx'akx'e/ wonder, astonishment maswi /majwi/ milk, sweet milk taoma /te'oma/ to hunt dikgwa /dikx'wa/ clumps of bush mmini i /mini/ dancer bonnye /bope/ smaller setshaba /setjhaba/ nation tlhatsa /tlhata'a/ vomit gotlha /xotiha/ rub a aurface aonyo /mopo/ dew; mist references baragwanath hospital, department of speech therapy and audiology. speech discrimination testing materials: zulu and sotho. soweto, 1977. beukelman, d.r. and yorkston, k.m. the relationship between information transfer and speech intelligibility of dysarthric patients. journal of communication disorders, 12, 189-196, 1979. black, j.w. accompaniments of word intelligibility. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 17, 409-418, 1952. black, j.w. multiple-choice intelligibility tests. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 22, 213-235, 1957. brown, j.t. setswana-english dictionary. third edition. botswana book centre (u.c.s.s.a.), 1971. cole, d.t. an introduction to tswana grammar. london: longmans, green and co., 1955. darley, f.l., aronson, a.e. and brown, j.r. motor speech disorders. london: saunders, 1975. doke, c.m. text-book of zulu grammar. second edition. london: longmans, green and co., 1931. doke, c.m. and mofokeng, s.m. textbook of southern sotho grammar. cape town: longmans, green and co., 1957 egan, j.p. articulation testing methods. laryngoscope, 58, 955991, 1948. fairbanks, g. test of phonemic differentiation: the rhyme test. journal of the acoustical society of america, 30(7), 596-600, 1958 fairbanks, g. voice and articulation drillbook. second edition, new york: harper and row, 1960. gowlett, d.f. and rassman, e.f.a. word-frequency count for southern sotho prose literature. african studies, 38(2), 107133, 1979. griesel, g.j. die invloed van 'n kwantitatiewe taaltonleding op leerstofseleksie by die onderrig van zoeloe as tweede taal. master's dissertation, university of natal, pietermaritzburg, 1979. griggs, t. from articulation tests towards formulas for intelligibility. language and speech, 1, 194-221, 1958. hillbrow hospital, department of speech therapy and audiology. phonetic inventories: zulu and southern sotho., johannesburg, 1981. hirsh, i.j., david, h. silverman, s.r., reynolds, e.g., eldert, e. and benson, r.w. development of materials for speech audiometry. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 17, 321-337, 1952. marlene carno jacobson and anthony traill hodson, b.w. and paden, e.p. targeting intelligible speech: a phonologic approach to remediation. san diego: collegehill press, 1982. hollen'oeck, a.r. problems of reliability in observational research. in sackett, g.p. (ed.) observing behaviour vol. ii; data collection and analysis methods. nichd mental retardation series, baltimore: university park press, 1978. jacobson, m.c. speech intelligibility and articulatory dynamics of reconstructed oral cancer patients. ph.d. thesis. university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, 1986. jordan, a.c. a practical course in xhosa. cape town: longmans, 1966. khumalo, s.j.m. zulu tonology. master's dissertation. university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, 1981. kipfmueller, l.t. and prins, t.d. consonant intelligibility: reliability and validity of a speech assessment procedure. journal of speech and hearing research, 14, 559-564, 1971. kunene, d.p. the ideophone in southern sotho. marburger studien zur afrika und asienkunde. berlin: verlag von dietrich reimer, 1978. kyle, j.g., conrad, r., mckenzie, m.g., morris, a.j.m. and weiskrantz, b.c. language abilities in deaf school leavers. teacher of the deaf, 2, 38-42, 1978. lanham, l.w. an outline history of the languages of southern africa. in l.w. lanham and k.p. prinsloo (eds.), language and communication studies in south africa. cape town: oxford university press, 1978. lehiste, i. and peterson, g.e. linguistic considerations in the study of speech intelligibility. journal of the acoustical society of america, 31(3), 280-286, 1959. mabille, a. and dieterlen, h. southern sotho-english dictionary. revised by paroz, r.a. revised and enlarged eighth edition. basutoland: morija, 1961. markides, α., assessing the speech intelligibility of hearing impaired children: oral reading versus picture description. teacher of the deaf, 2, 185-189, 1978. meyerson, m.d., johnson, b.h. and weitzman, r.s. rehabilitation of a patient with complete mandibulectomy and partial glossectomy. american journal of otolaryngology, 1, 256-261, 1980. miller, g.a., heise, g.a. and lichten, w. the intelligibility of speech as a function of the context of the test materials. journal of experimental psychology, 41, 329-335, 1951. nichols, a.c. confusions in recognising phonemes spoken by esophageal speakers: i. initial consonants and clusters. journal of communication disorders, 9, 27-41, 1976. piatt, l.j., andrews, g., young, m. and neilson, p.e). the measurement of speech impairment of adults with cerebral palsy. folia phoniatrica, 30, 50-58, 1978. i prather, e.m. scaling defectiveness of articulation by direct magnitude estimation. journal of speech and hearing research, 3(4), 380-392, 1960. rosenbek, j.c. and lapointe, l.l. the dysarthrias: description, diagnosis and treatment. in d.f. johns (ed.), clinical management of neurogenic communicative disorders. boston: little, brown, 1978. sackett, g.p. measurement in observational research. in g.p. sackett (ed.), observing behaviour, vol. ii: data collection and analysis methods. nichd mental retardation series. baltimore: university park press, 1978. / schiavetti, n., metz, d.e. and sitler, r.w. construct validity of direct magnitude estimation and interval scaling of speech intelligibility: evidence from a study of the hearing-impaired. journal of speech and hearing research, 24, 441-445, 1981. schiavetti, n., sitler, r.w., metz, d.e. and houde, r.a. prediction of contextual speech intelligibility from isolated (continued on page 28) / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) v new automatic hearing aid a truly advanced and innovative sound amplifier the automatic sound processor (asp) was developed especially for those people who have difficulty in hearing speech in noisy environments. for years, hearing aid wearers have avoided restaurants, theatres, church and parties because of the inability of an ordinary hearing aid to control background noises. the a.s.p. is simply and uniquely designed to normalize background noise so that speech is more easily ' understood. * to be able to restore sound to even one human being makes your l life especially important. to restore hearing... what a blessed talent to have medifix (pty) ltd. surgical & medical cape p.o. box 19 , p.o. box 52 bedfordview lynedoch 2008 7603 south africa phone: (02234) 442 phone: (oil) 53-4188/9 or (02231)93442 /7λν natal p.o. box 47443 greyville 4023 phone: (031) 236164 | r | r i c h a r d s hearing systems tomorrow's technology for today's hearing problem r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 (continued from page 26) word intelligibility measures. journal f speech and hearing research 27, 623-626, 1984. schmidt s intelligibility and the child with multiple articulation deviations. in h.a. winitz (ed.), treating articulation disorders: for clinicians by clinicians. baltimore: university park press, 1984. schultz, m.c. world familiarity influences in speech discrimination. journal of speech and hearing research, 7, 395-400. shriberg, l.d. and kwiatkowski, j. phonological disorders iii: a procedure for assessing severity of involvement. journal of speech and hearing disorders. 47, 242-256, 1982. skelly, m. (ed.) glossectomee speech rehabilitation. springfield, illinois, charles c. thomas, 1973. south africa 1985: official yearbook of the republic of south africa. eleventh edition. johannesburg: chris van rensburg, 1985. tikofsky, r.s. a revised list for the estimation of dysarthric single word intelligibiliy. journal of speech and hearing research, 13, 59-64, 1970. tobias, j.v. on phonemic analysis of speech discrimination tests. journal of speech and hearing research, 7, 98-100, 1964. van wyk, e.b. language contact and bilingualism. in l.w. lanham and k.p. prinsloo (eds.), language and communication studies in south africa. cape town: oxford university press, 1978. westphal, e.o.j. syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, 20, 22-41, 1973. marlene carno jacobson and anthony traill wilkes, a. bantu language studies. in l.w. lanham and k.p. prinsloo (eds.), language and communication in south africa, cape town: oxford university press, 1978. yorkston, k.m. and beukelman, d.r. a comparison of techniques for measuring intelligibility of dysarthric speech. journal of communication disorders, 499-512, 1978. yorkston, k.m. and beukelman, d.r. a. a clinician-judged technique for quantifying dysarthric speech based on single word intelligibility. journal of communication disorders, 13, 15-31 1980. yorkston, k.m. and beukelman, d.r. communication efficiency of dysarthric speakers as measured by sentence intelligibility and speaking rate. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 46, 296-301, 1981. young, m.a. and downs. t.d. testing the significance of the agreement among observers. journal of speech and hearing research, 11, 5-17, 1968. ziervogel, d., louw, j.α., ferreira, j.α., baumbach, e.j.m. and lombard, d.p. handbook of the speech sounds and sound changes of the bantu languages of south africa, unisa handbook series no. 3e. university of south africa, pretoria, 1967. ziervogel, d., louw, j.a. and taljaard, p.c. a handbook of the zulu language. third edition. pretoria: j.l. van schaik, 1981. ziervogel, d. and mokgokong, p.c. groot noord-sotho woordeboek. pretoria: j.l. van schaik, 1975. kritiese beoordeling van evaluasietegnieke van toepassing op gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas brenda louw, d.phil (pretoria) isabel c. uys, d.phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria, pretoria. opsomming die verkryging van 'n klinies geldige en 'n voorskrywende proflel van 'n baba se kommunikasiefunksionering verg noukeurige seleksie van ondersoekmetodes en meetinstrumente wat aangewend word. voorgestelde evaluasietegnieke wat van toepassing is op swart gesplete-lip-enverhemeite-babas word teen die agtergrond van 'n kritiese oorsig van die evaluasieproses beoordeel in terme van navorsingsen kliniese toepassingswaarde. vyf-en-twintig swart babas met gesplete verhemelte onder die ouderdom van twaalf maande het asproefpersone gedien. riglyne vir toepassing van die voorgestelde evaluasiemodel deur middel van direkte dienslewering deur die spraakterapeut en deur middel van konsultasie-as-voorkoming in 'n multi-kulturele gemeenskap word verskaf. / abstract the obtaining of a clinically valid and prescriptive profile of the communication function of infants calls for an accurate selection of the research methods and measuring instruments to be used. suggested evaluation techniques applicable to black cleft lip and palate infants are judged and evaluated within the framework of a critical review of the evaluation process in terms of research and clinical applicability. twenty-five black cleft lip and palate infants under the age of twelve months served as subjects. guidelines are supplied for use of these techniques through direct services provided by the speech pathologist and through consultation -asprevention in a multi-cultural society. © sasha 1986 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) teacher support an exploration.html teacher support – an exploration of how foundation-phase teachers facilitate language skills anna-marie wium department communication pathology, university of pretoria brenda louw professor emeritus: department communication pathology, university of pretoria correspondence to: a wium (anna-marie1.wium@ul.ac.za) abstract the role of speech-language therapists (slts) has been redefined by white paper 6, which emphasises the role of support to both teachers and learners. slts have expert knowledge and skills pertaining to communication and language, and therefore have much to contribute to the process of learning in teaching. this article builds on a previous article published in the 2010 edition of the journal, which reported on the process of supporting teachers to facilitate listening, language and numeracy skills in semi-rural and urban (township) contexts. in this follow-up article the focus is on the qualitative findings obtained from a specific section of the larger study. where the overall study made use of a mixed methods approach to evaluate the process of providing support, and reported on the entire continued professional development (cpd) programme, this article focuses specifically on the qualitative data collected when the cpd programme addressed the facilitation of language. this article explores how the strategies were used in the classrooms, and the benefits of the support provided. the data discussed in this article were obtained from questionnaires, focus groups, and critical self-evaluation by teachers, as well as a research diary used by the programme facilitator. the results show that both the participants and their learners benefited from the support provided. the participants reportedly for the first time were able to meet curriculum outcomes which previously had been omitted, and showed an increased ability to plan their lessons. several teachers experienced changes in their teaching practices and could reflect on their practices, which contributed to their professional development. these teachers became more empowered. learning in the classroom was enhanced through increased participation of all learners, and enjoyment of the strategies. keywords: language, literacy, collaboration, numeracy, teachers, support, speech-language therapist speech-language therapists (slts) have expert knowledge and skills regarding language acquisition and literacy, which place them in a position to support teachers with the implementation of the revised national curriculum statement (rncs) (department of education, 2002). this article builds on a paper published in the 2010 edition of this journal (wium, louw & eloff, 2010) which focused on the development of a continued professional development (cpd) programme researched using a mixed methods approach. the focus was on the process of supporting teachers to facilitate listening, language and numeracy skills, whereas this article concentrates specifically on the language component of the overall study. this article explores how the teachers facilitated language skills in their classrooms as a result of strategies learnt in a cpd programme, and how they experienced the support provided. such findings provide guidelines to slts who have to provide support to teachers in schools. this article firstly discusses the collaborative roles of slts in education contexts and the interrelationship between listening, language and literacy. such information was used to develop the workshop material to support teachers in the facilitation of language skills as described by the rncs. the content of the cpd programme is briefly described in appendix a. background in south africa the performance of learners in literacy and numeracy is alarmingly poor. the implication is that the majority of learners in south africa are currently not receiving quality education, which can be considered as a violation of their constitutional rights. attempts to facilitate literacy and numeracy learning, particularly in the early grades, need to be improved. research by girolametto, weitzman, lefebvre and greenberg (2007) indicated that many teachers in care centres in the usa lack the knowledge to facilitate emergent literacy skills. such findings may also apply to the south african context, as formal qualifications for teachers of grade r learners have not been a requirement until 2011 (motshekga, 2010). the need for teacher support in the implementation of the curriculum has become a national priority (department of education, 2008; motshekga, 2010), which slts can provide by using a collaborative approach, particularly in the literacy learning area. collaborative roles of the slt in education contexts white paper 6 (department of education, 2001) outlines the government’s strategy to transform the current education system to make it more efficient, equitable and just. this document also specifies that slts should play consultative and collaborative roles in district and school-based support teams and support both teachers and learners who experience barriers to learning. white paper 6 requires slts to provide training, mentoring, monitoring, and consultation to facilitate literacy and numeracy. with regard to literacy, teachers need to understand the complex nature of language and also how to facilitate it in the classroom. the support of young learners who experience barriers to learning is essential because communication is central to the social, emotional, and academic development of young children (department of education, 2008). in terms of supporting learners slts should focus on the prevention of communication disorders (including literacy development problems), and provide language programmes in schools for the whole classroom as a group. such preventive strategies are aligned with recommendations made by the american speech and hearing association (asha) (2001), which require slts to provide foundation-phase learners (grades k 3) with suitable intervention for literacy development and to address reading and writing skills in older learners. child language development is an interdisciplinary field of knowledge that is shared by teachers and slts because language is the foundation for developing competence in reading, writing, listening, and speaking. language deficits may delay the acquisition of these four modes of communication, resulting in barriers to learning (owens, 2004). teachers and slts should work as a collaborative team to prevent and overcome such barriers, and to share their knowledge and skills to this effect. teachers are primarily responsible for the teaching of reading and writing, whereas slts attend to the cross-modal literacy-language connection between all four modes of language, as these may affect one another. in south africa many learners have to acquire oral and written language skills in their home language as well as english (motshekga, 2010); this implies that learners who require support in the development of language will need to be supported in both their home language and in english. the inter-relationship between listening, language, literacy and numeracy language is the foundation for learning (owens, 2004), and is an integral part of ‘literacy’ in the foundation-phase curriculum (motshekga, 2010). the four language systems shown in table i are integrated in the rncs as listening, speaking, reading, viewing, writing, thinking, and reasoning, as well as language structure and use (department of education, 2002). each of these language systems is associated with either receptive or expressive modes of communication (johnson & roseman, 2003). language is not restricted to the oral modality, but also includes the visual modality (johnson & roseman, 2003). learners developing written language awareness discover that print is a highly organised system that reflects oral language and guides them to an understanding of the alphabetic principle (justice & ezell, 2002), which relates to literacy in the rncs. the national curriculum: literacy in order for learners to develop language and communication skills (department of education, 2002), they firstly have to listen attentively and respond critically to information. the rncs requires learners to communicate confidently and effectively in a spoken language in a wide range of situations. it is essential that learners learn to read and view information for enjoyment, and respond critically to the aesthetic, cultural and emotional values in texts. the rncs requires learners to read and write different kinds of texts, but also to use language to think and reason. learners are required to become competent in language structure and use and in doing so they have to use the sounds, words, and the grammar of a language to create and interpret texts. the development of language and communication skills is therefore critical for effective learning. provided that the curriculum has been properly implemented, learners should be able to read and write and perform basic mathematical calculations by the end of grade 3. an outcomes-based education (obe) approach is integral to the rncs. it requires a skills-based, problem-solving, co-operative approach to teaching and learning. teachers firstly have to decide on which skills and concepts they would like their learners to acquire, and then create suitable contexts in which such skills can be facilitated in the classroom. within an obe approach teachers plan in teams in order to equip learners with similar skills across the grade. such group planning ensures that the standard of education is the same, and teachers need to agree on what to teach and the activities required (department of education, 2008). learners from low socio-economic schools (ses) require a variety of experiences to facilitate the natural transition from oral language used at home to functional literate language used in school. children living in poverty are at risk for learning disorders, and need support. the current context education in south africa is complex, as several challenges are encountered across contexts by both teachers and learners (rembe, 2005). the poor performance of learners in south africa can often be attributed to the fact that 40% of children in south africa come from extremely impoverished backgrounds with limited access to learner support materials in their homes. access to printed material in shared reading experiences, as well as parental beliefs about literacy, have been identified as having an effect on writing. learners raised in poor communities mostly have limited exposure to printed material and subsequently may have very different attitudes to, and experiences of, the printed text from those of their peers (nancollis, lawrie, & dodd, 2005). locke, ginsborg and peers (2002) reported that preschool children who were raised in impoverished environments in the uk performed at lower levels in oral language assessments than the general population, which put them at risk for delayed written language skills. learners from low ses often experience difficulty in making the shift from the language used at home to the abstract and decontextualised language used in the classroom (justice & kaderavek, 2004). these learners may therefore require more support than their counterparts (department of education, 2008). inadequate oral language development may result in poor academic performance (figure 1), which points to a link between language and literacy. with reference to figure 1, emergent literacy involves both written language awareness and phonological awareness (justice & ezell, 2001), which in turn are based on normal oral language (particularly vocabulary development). in turn, age-appropriate oral language development is required for the development of reading competence, and therefore oral language proficiency is regarded as predictive of reading achievements as well as other written language achievements at a later stage. figure 1 shows that adequate print-related language (e.g. familiarity with books and visual symbols) is required for continued oral language development (justice, skibbe, & ezell, 2006). a similar reciprocal relationship exists between phonological awareness and reading, as each facilitates and is facilitated by the other (justice, skibbe & ezell, 2006). learners’ language learning is a crucial precursor to literacy. poor literacy development contributes to later problems in language. the link between language and literacy language is essential for the acquisition of literacy and numeracy because it is the foundation for speaking, reading, writing, and spelling. for emergent literacy to develop, learners need to firstly develop meta-linguistic skills (johnson & roseman, 2003) to identify and analyse specific sounds to allow them to read or write. phonological development (including phonological awareness) (figure 2) provides the bridge between language and literacy whereas higher-level phonological skills (e.g. sound manipulation and substitution) facilitate written language development in terms of reading and spelling (johnson & roseman, 2003). similarly, adequate language development is required to facilitate the language required for numeracy. the american speech hearing association (asha)’s position statement (2001: 16) advocates that ‘... children need to experience reading, spelling, and writing for authentic communication purposes in which vocabulary, grammar, and discourse skills converge.’ learners who do not have adequate and age-appropriate listening and language skills when entering formal education may be at risk for academic failure (justice & kaderavek, 2004). this, in turn, may cause problems such as low self-esteem, social maladjustment, and inability to support themselves financially. it is therefore important to prevent academic failure by ensuring that learners acquire such skills as early as possible to allow them to become academically competitive when going to school. lessing and de wit (2008) were of the opinion that the teachers’ own lack of conceptual knowledge of language and the sub-skills required for literacy acquisition were at the root of their use of outdated teaching practices such as rote learning (e.g. drilling and chanting). such outdated teaching practices do not facilitate learning. teachers should aim instead for the development of meta-linguistic skills, which are required for learners to identify and analyse specific sounds to allow them to read or write (johnson & roseman, 2003). it appears that learners from the most disadvantaged homes may be further challenged by the inadequate teaching practices prevalent in their classrooms. teachers need to be supported to develop an understanding of the underlying concepts of language for learning, and to develop strategies and skills to facilitate the four language systems included in the rncs in order for learners to develop literacy skills. the specific cpd programme was part of a research study that covered several topics, and this particular article focuses on the language component of the entire research project. the topics were repeated in two contexts over a period of 2 years within an action research approach (stringer, 2007). such support was based on a three-pronged approach that consisted of a workshop (training component), the implementation of the skills in the classroom (practical component), and a mentoring component which provided the teachers with feedback on lesson plans and portfolio assignments (wium, louw, & eloff, 2010). the results obtained demonstrate how the facilitative strategies (e.g. the use of stories, songs, and art within a theme) were used to facilitate language. these activities were combined with reading and writing activities in the classroom, which were submitted as portfolios. method aim of the research and objectives the aim of the research was to describe the outcomes of a particular cpd programme for foundation-phase teachers for the facilitation of language skills. to this end the research focused on how the participants (teachers) facilitated language development in their classrooms following the support provided, and how the participants valued the support provided to themselves and their learners. in this article the term ‘participant’ refers to the teachers who participated in the research, and ‘learners’ to the learners in classrooms (grades r 3). study design an action research (stringer, 2007) approach was used to determine how the participants facilitated language in their classrooms as a result of a specific cpd programme. this section of the research was originally included as part of the overall project that performed programme evaluation with mixed methods research (wium, et al., 2010). each research cycle collected data with questionnaires prior to and following the workshops, which was followed by a period of practical implementation with the completion of portfolios. these portfolios displayed evidence of lesson planning and practical implementation of strategies in the classrooms, as well as self-reflection. at the conclusion of each cycle a focus group was conducted to evaluate the support provided, as well as the implementation of the strategies learnt. this research cycle was repeated in both contexts. throughout each cycle the programme facilitator continually reflected on the entire process in a research diary. the context as mentioned in the previous article (wium, et al., 2010), the research was repeated in two contexts: a semi-rural context and an urban (townships and informal settlements) context. many learners in south africa are educated in a language which is not their first language, or by teachers who speak a different language from the language of learning and teaching (lolt) (wium, 2010). the research was conducted in a context where northern sotho is the dominant language, which explains why 63% of the participants (n=96) used this language as home language (l1). the other major languages represented as being the participants’ l1 included tswana (11%), isizulu (11%), and to a lesser extent other official african languages of south africa (excluding english and afrikaans). of the teachers 61% used northern sotho as the lolt, whereas 33% used english as lolt in the semi-rural context compared with 25% in the urban township context. in both these contexts setswana and isizulu were used as lolts to a lesser extent. the cpd programme was presented in english as it is the language used for support by the gauteng department of education (gde), and also the language used in higher education (ministry of education, 2001). the aim of the cpd programme was to make the participants aware of the language skills required for learning, and to provide them with strategies to facilitate language development in the lolt in relation to the rncs. participants the sampling process is discussed according to the criteria for selection, selection procedure and sample size. criteria for section of the participants the selection of the schools to be supported was determined by the gde as they aimed to redress past inequities. all participants included in this study were required to meet the following criteria: • all had to be appointed full-time in teaching positions in the foundation phase (grades r, 1, 2 and 3) at schools in the targeted contexts. • only teachers who wanted to participate of their own free will were included. they were made aware that participation was voluntary and that they should not have been coerced by their superiors or peers. those teachers who did not want to participate in the programme were excluded. • participants were expected to feel comfortable with the use of english as medium of instruction. an introductory letter of invitation to the schools alerted the teachers to the fact that the cpd programme would be provided in english, which allowed them to make informed decisions with regard to their participation in the research. selection procedure twelve schools from a semi-rural area and 12 schools from an urban/densely populated area (including township schools and schools from informal settlements) were selected by the gde. a total number of 24 low ses in the tshwane region were targeted for this project over a period of 2 consecutive years. • each of the selected schools was required to identify one teacher from each grade level in the foundation phase who had volunteered to participate. the intention was for these participants to go back to their schools to share their knowledge and skills with the wider community. • a similar procedure of volunteerism was used for compiling the focus groups. by using a nested design, the participants in the focus groups were already included in the original sample. only one representative from each school was required to participate in the focus groups. sample size each grade level (grades r 3) was represented by 12 participants, totalling 48 per annum. the entire sample consisted of 96 participants, which was considered sufficient to serve the purpose of this specific study and was representative of foundation-phase teachers in both contexts (leedy & ormrod, 2010). as only one primary programme facilitator was available to conduct the workshops, a group of 48 participants per workshop was regarded as manageable. this number was considered sufficient to allow for possible attrition later in the programme. all the participants in the semi-rural context were female, whereas two of the participants in the urban context were male. the focus groups each consisted of 12 participants (one representing each school per context), as this number is considered an adequate size for this purpose. it was also a representative sample (25%) of the entire group that was trained, and allowed for attrition. sampling method the sample was selected by means of stratified random sampling, which is a probability sampling method (leedy & ormrod, 2010). each participating school identified one individual from the list of volunteers in each grade level of the foundation phase (e.g. grades r, 1, 2, and 3), so that four participants from each school enrolled for the programme. the qualitative strand obtained data from the entire sample (96) with questionnaires and portfolio assessments, but also used a nested design (onwuegbuzie & dickinson, 2007) for the focus group. the participants who volunteered to participate in the focus groups originated from the entire sample and were therefore similar to those in the rest of the study. the sample was fairly homogeneous in terms of contexts, grade levels represented, and the teachers’ experience in teaching, although not in terms of qualifications and language, and can therefore be considered as a cross-section of the population (leedy & ormrod, 2010). data collection methods the researchers wanted to understand how the participants implemented the strategies, as well as their impression of the benefits obtained from the support provided. this required various forms of qualitative data (leedy & ormrod, 2010). data collection material data were obtained from open-ended questions via questionnaires, focus groups, critical reflection in portfolios, and a research diary completed by the researcher. open-ended questions allowed respondents to express themselves freely and to make suggestions. although open-ended questions were useful to obtain additional information that could add to the understanding of phenomena, they were kept to a minimum as they take longer to complete and therefore could be a cause of non-response. the questions, instructions and layout of the questionnaires were formulated based on guidelines obtained from the literature (mcmillan & schumacher, 2010). a language editor reviewed and edited the questions, and two experts in the professional field, as well as a statistical advisor, scrutinised the various questionnaires to ascertain their validity as a measuring instrument, and to identify any potentially imprecise or ambiguous terms. pretesting determined the clarity of instructions as well as questions, and the time for completion. focus groups were used to evaluate the workshop in terms of the participants’ impressions/feelings about the workshop, and to obtain their experiences in implementing the strategies. in addition, information was obtained on how the participants regarded their own individual levels of skill in implementing the strategies at the end of the 3-week implementation period. the focus group schedule was compiled according to specific criteria obtained from the literature. categories of questions included opening questions, introductory questions, transition questions, key questions, ending questions, and ‘putting the parts together’. the focus group plan was reviewed with experts and then pilot tested prior to use. two experts in the professional fields of slt and education scrutinised the schedules to assess the expected responses, which increased the likelihood of both content and construct validity. the portfolio assessments were used to evaluate the participants’ applied knowledge and to monitor the implementation of strategies. it was assumed that implementation of the strategies learnt would increase the participants’ competence in planning their lessons and facilitate language for learning. the aim of the research diary was to document the research process and to reflect on issues arising. it provided insight regarding factors that could affect the outcomes of the programme. data entries were used to share ideas on the process with experts and colleagues, but also to observe ‘real world’ processes. questions could be answered about methods used. such continued reflection resulted in changes being made; therefore this process could be associated with evidence-based research. data collection process the data used in this article were obtained from questionnaires, self-reflection sheets in portfolios, focus groups and diary entries in a research journal. questionnaires were handed to participants prior to and following training, and were collected by hand after completion. the four participants from each school were required to implement the strategies in the classrooms for a period of at least 3 weeks following the workshops. at the end of the implementation period, they were required to engage in self-reflection (guided reflection) and to include the self-reflection sheets in their portfolios. the focus groups met within 4 6 weeks of the workshop to establish the value of the learning experience and to monitor the implementation of the strategies taught. in addition, diary entries were made throughout the process to reflect on the process. such diary entries were included as data. credibility all the qualitative data analysed were reviewed by inter-rater agreement of coding with 80% accuracy. focus groups were conducted in both contexts. at the end of each focus group meeting, the assistant moderator verbally summarised the responses to questions (as documented from the completed summary sheet). member checking was done immediately when these summaries were presented to the group for approval, thereby increasing the trustworthiness of the data. after the participants had departed, the researcher and the assistant moderator met to reflect on the procedures, the participation, and outcomes of the session. they compared notes and confirmed the key ideas. the researcher further reflected on the focus group shortly afterwards with entries in the research diary. thick descriptions within the context were created and rich data from several data sources (diary entries, focus groups, questionnaires and portfolio reflections) were triangulated, which increased the credibility. data analyses content analysis (qualitative research) provided a clear description of classroom practices and the experiences of the trainees following the support provided (leedy & ormrod, 2010). the focus group transcriptions, diary entries and open-ended questions from questionnaires were qualitatively analysed with atlas-ti (thomas muir scientific software development, 2003 2004) (qualitative descriptive analysis). such software allowed for counting of the occurrence of the codes (enumeration) to determine the prominence thereof. items coded were then grouped as categories and themes within the logic model framework of input, process, output and outcomes. where the overall project reported previously (wium, et al., 2010) focused on the process of providing support, this article concentrates on the outcomes of the component that facilitated language skills. in the overall study all items coded were quantitised by using a binary scale to categorise the data as either positive or negative (supporting the item, or refuting it), which allowed a comparison of quantitative and qualitative data within a mixed methods approach. the interpretation of the inferences was subjected to a validation process before final conclusions could be drawn. results and discussion the binary classification used to interpret the data in the overall project is used in this article as it reflects how the strategies were implemented in the classroom, and the benefits obtained from the support provided. in terms of the former, the following themes can be identified: increased competence in meeting curriculum outcomes, working in themes, and language issues. the benefits of the cpd programme are discussed in terms of the benefits to the teachers (e.g. improvement in lesson planning, working in a theme and language issues), and for the learners (e.g. increased participation of all learners, and enjoyment). selected extracts and quotes obtained from the qualitative data are provided in italics in the discussion of the results. implementation of strategies in the classroom meeting curriculum outcomes the benefits obtained from the support by the teachers included an increased ability to address the curriculum outcomes through the implementation of specific strategies. perceptions of increased competence with the implementation of strategies to facilitate language learning became evident from the following quotes: ‘i took many things out of that story. i made a song, made a poem, and then they must do the plurals, the opposites, segmentation, and then i also stated the new vocabulary. it takes maybe two weeks … on one story. which is [why] i forgot about the assessment.’ (line 28, focus group 1) ‘that any story can teach learners all the learning outcomes.’ (line 20, reflection and self-evaluation of teachers) the use of stories allowed the participants to integrate various assessment standards within a single activity, which is in accordance with the principles of obe. the support also integrated literacy with other learning areas, e.g. life skills, where values such as respect for animals could be taught. ‘a told us how much the story has made an impact on her class. previously she taught numeracy through counting (rote counting). now she makes sure that the story introduces the numbers and concepts within a more meaningful manner (line 22, diary entry 18, pilot focus 2) t: ‘when we tell the story, animals (some learners do not respect animals), when i tell them about animals; they see that they have to respect the animals.’ f: ‘was that because of the story or why did they learn to respect animals?’ t: ‘the story that i was telling – they have changed. i think they have changed.’ (line 42, pilot focus group) in both contexts it became evident during role-play activities conducted in the workshops that some participants at first did not clearly understand how to construct a story or how to hold the attention of learners when reading a storybook. this may be because either they have not used this strategy before, or possibly because having to use english (which was an additional language for all participants) in the role play activity, which could have inhibited their ability to express themselves freely. in general, the participants reported that the use of a story with pictures, as well as book reading, yield satisfactory results as their learners were able to listen attentively and to retell the story. the inference is that the implementation of the story and the use of pictures enhanced the learners’ receptive and expressive language skills. t: and even that one of … the sequencing. when i was just telling them the story, so that they listen and then afterwards, they could tell the story. they were able to sit and listen and then afterwards they could tell us the sequence.’ (line 46, focus group 1) support in lesson planning support in lesson planning had particular value to the participants. prior to the support provided, many of the participants did not understand the value of integrating various activities around a central theme in order to enrich the learners’ conceptual language base and understanding of vocabulary. the participants agreed that using a central theme helped them to plan their lessons. t1 ‘yes it helped me with planning of the lesson.’ (line 191, focus group 3 (b)) t2: ‘most of our teachers had problems with planning our lessons. or creating los. i am so perfect now. i can now use the one lo and apply it to another – we kill two los.’ (line 284, pilot focus group 1) strategies to be used within a central theme, e.g. stories and role play, relate to the functional approach to language learning and increase linguistic awareness (owens, 2004). working within a central theme in general the songs and nursery rhymes supported and expanded vocabulary pertaining to the original theme of the story, and highlighted semantic and syntactic forms (paul, 2007). songs and rhymes supported by transparent gestures or accompanied by movements, as well as role play, allowed for repetition of vocabulary, but also provided the opportunity for multimodal experiences that could facilitate learning. such strategies provided a ‘script’ for learning language, as learners were encouraged to fill in parts that have purposefully been left out once the learners have become familiarised with the story, song, or rhyme. ‘i was thinking that if all the teachers were attending workshops like these, lots of things were going to change at our schools – involving the negative attitudes of teachers for learners who have barriers, and teachers themselves who don’t realise that they are barriers themselves for the learners. because they don’t want to apply new strategies in their lessons.’ (refer to hu 46, line 33. testimonials of learner support teachers) a few participants complained that they found it difficult to match the story with a rhyme and/or a song within a specific theme. they complained about how difficult it had been to design a good story that encompassed all the different elements stipulated in the assignment. (line 17 in diary entry 18, focus group 2) it is possible that the participants had followed a fragmented approach in the past where such activities were conducted in isolation, as was the case with the previous transmission approach to learning. in this case these participants may have benefited from more peer support or mentoring. a central theme was instrumental in the creation of a meaningful context that facilitated understanding and allowed for the use of a variation of intervention activities. themes allowed the learners in class to incorporate new learning into existing frameworks and to gain familiarity with concepts (allowing them to express these in language), as well as to develop understanding. apart from providing activities for listening and speaking, teachers were required to encourage reading and writing within the general theme of the week. the use of a theme integrated the thread of language throughout the curriculum in all classroom activities. such activities within specific themes allowed for cultural diversity and various learning styles, and therefore created an optimal learning environment for learners. language issues some of the participants reported that the use of prepositions was difficult to implement when the lolt was an african language. they explained how they made use of different ways to express the use of prepositions. t: ‘i also struggled, so i looked at the story and tried to implement the strategies. but some of the things we do not do in n sotho. like … prepositions, and … adjectives!’ (line 97, focus group 2) t: ‘we say ka-ga-re (inside), kamorago (behind). e-kamogare. e-mogauswe, e-kamorage (sing-song style).’ (line 109, focus group 2) these participants tended to use archiforms (e.g. use of one member of a word class to represent all members) to refer to several positions in space and augmented the meaning with different hand gestures. such use of prepositions relates to the typical language use of additional language speakers, although in this case archiforms were used by some of the participants when communicating with learners in their l1. some participants reported on their learners’ limited vocabulary, which did not include a wide range of prepositions, and that learners often use a single preposition to represent several others. they reported that they refer to positions in space in a similar manner as their learners do, because they do not expect their learners to understand them if they express themselves differently. such practices did not allow for conceptual growth or for an expansion of vocabulary and therefore these participants’ lack of insight and/or limited proficiency in the lolt could be regarded as barriers to learning. the importance of language modelling (paul, 2007) needs to be emphasised in future programmes because learners need an adult as ‘knowledgeable other’ (in this case the teacher) to provide them with the relevant insights within cultural and social exchange. it should be noted that these teachers’ limited insight into what language consists of and how it can be facilitated can be attributed to several reasons which are rooted in the south african context (e.g. inadequate prior training of teachers who were trained under the previous dispensation, and the lack of formal qualifications). increased insight in meta-language was noted as some participants also complained that subject-specific vocabulary and terminology do not necessarily exist in indigenous languages, which required of them to explain such concepts through the use of gestures, or by making use of more elaborate descriptions. f: ‘but then you explain it with gestures? you can also explain “kagare” as being “behind”?’ (line 109, focus group 2) benefits of the programme the inferences indicated that 95% (n=288) of all items coded in terms of the benefits of the programme were positive, but these results are discussed separately as benefits to the participants and the learners. benefits of the cpd programme to the participants the results indicated that 96% of the 137 items coded were positive; this included the participants’ perception of changes that occurred in their teaching practices, and their ability to reflect on their practices, as well as their empowerment. ‘it has empowered me enormously and i am highly skilled to deal with learners’ problems with sound-right strategies, and confident to approach any learning problem and to assist my colleagues with pride.’ (line 128, un-tabled open questions, forms 2 & 3) evidence of ‘empowerment’ (n=17) is related to the fourth of five levels of knowledge acquisition described by miller & watts (1990), where learners become knowledgeable to the extent of training their peers. in this case, it resulted in some of the participants training their colleagues (and was therefore coded as ‘training of others’). coenders, terlouw and dijkstra (2008) reported on the successful preparation of teachers for a new science curriculum by having them develop and use curriculum materials, as it created ownership and strengthened their pedagogical content knowledge (pck). even though a small sample (n=7) was used in their study, these findings resonate with findings in this study where teachers had to prepare lesson plans for assessment. moreover, as the participants came to realise that they all shared similar problems, a network of support was established between schools. they also came to realise that others are in the same boat, and that they need to support one another as teachers. networking was also established (line 42, diary entry 25). a sense of collegiality appeared to have developed between the participants through sharing experiences, which verifies the value of group and peer learning. benefit to the learners the effect of the programme on the learners is described by information obtained from secondary data on participants’ perceptions of the effect of the strategies on their learners. in general the participants were positive (94%, n=132) about the effect the strategies had on their learners, which is promising as gilmore and vance (2007) found a positive correlation between teachers’ overall rating of attentive listening and learners’ verbal comprehension test scores. research to determine the impact of programmes on learners’ performance is limited. the current study reported perceived gains made by learners, but these findings were subjective. the effect of cpd programmes on learners’ performance needs further investigation. increased participation of all learners all the participants (100%, n=34) testified to the increased ‘participation of the learners’ when using the newly acquired strategies and activities, especially from learners who had been excluded in the past or would not participate. they particularly reported how learners participated in songs and listened to, and retold the stories. ‘learners can tell the stories with the pictures. even the learners who struggle, they can tell the story.’ (line 35, focus group 1) the learners were all able to retell the story and to participate in the songs using gestures and actions. such participation in these activities allowed learners to sufficiently internalise the language to eventually participate through the verbal medium. enjoyment of lessons a particular attribute of the programme was the element of ‘enjoyment’ that was experienced (100%, n=19) across contexts, and is illustrated in figure 3. because the learners enjoyed the new activities and participated in the classroom, the participants (teachers) responded positively and expressed their excitement about the outcomes. enjoyment of learning experiences facilitated learning in both the learners and the participants. ‘these strategies provide the language development. the classes are so much fun … sometimes i look at my class and i cannot believe the difference. the children, they all enjoy the lessons so much. sometimes i feel as if i just want to cry.’ (line 46, diary entry 16 on focus group 1) the continued reflection by both the participants and the researcher on the entire process led to their professional and personal growth. the results indicated that the strategies for language facilitation were experienced as positive (83% of the items coded, n=18). a summary of the results with regard to the implementation of the strategies and the benefits of the support provided are summarised in table ii. negative findings negative findings were related to the ‘process’ component of implementing the cpd programme, particularly the use of portfolio assignments with lesson planning, which were met with resistance as it added to the participants’ workload and put them under pressure. t: ‘implementation is very good, the problem is this assignment. to know … to write it. but it helps us. it really helps us. when we start planning again for those … or your … compiling everything. but i don’t like the assignment.’ (line 12, focus group 2) the participants also complained about time constraints in completing the portfolio assignments or implementing the strategies. such complaints were attributed to busy schedules, high workload, and low intrinsic motivation, which could be related to limited support from the school or a negative school culture. t: ‘yeah, because of lack of time. we have been so busy.’ (line 303, focus group 1) t: ‘in the week it is difficult. i think we should work on it for another two weeks.’ (line 161, focus group 1) t1: ‘it has been so hectic, since the schools closing.’ t2: ‘busy, very busy.’ a-m: ‘with what?’ t: ‘with meetings, some of the workshops.’ (line 15, focus group 2) sustainability of the cpd programme this cpd programme was one example of support provided to teachers and the question remains whether it had any long-term effect as it has not yet been researched. the complex nature of education as a contested context (mcmillan & schumacher, 2010; motshekga, 2010) requires a better understanding from slts working in the education environment (o’connor & geiger, 2009). informal feedback received from the district facilitators 2 years after their training suggested that the lesson-planning format, where groups of grade-level teachers planned their lessons together around a central theme, was still practised in some of the schools, possibly because an integrated thematic approach is supported by the gde as it is part of an obe approach. once, when working in a different context from the research, the programme facilitator encountered members of learner support teams (lsts) (who did not attend the original workshops), using the workshop handout as a resource, which indicated that the strategies and materials were discussed and shared with other colleagues. the message has spread to a wider community, which had been the original intention from the start, but this could also cause problems and be limiting. an initial introduction to the facilitation of language by means of a workshop, as well as follow-up support, is required to ensure quality and generalisation of principles. teacher development is not a ‘quick fix’ for existing problems, but is an ongoing process over time. information and skills need to be repeated and reflected on continually to bring about behaviour change. each learning experience provides a scaffold for future learning. while this cpd programme yielded positive benefits the long-term outcomes remain unknown. limitations of the research in terms of data collection, high levels of non-response were evident in the open-ended questions in the questionnaires, as well as in the critical reflections included in the portfolio assessments. training venues more central to the schools could have limited late arrivals and subsequent non-response in questionnaires. factors such as participants not being familiar with reflective practices, limited language proficiency in english, literacy levels, timing and logistics could have contributed to non-response in portfolio assessment. in some instances a negative school culture impacted on the participants’ motivation to complete the portfolio assignments. it is further acknowledged that working in close proximity with teachers over a prolonged period of time could have increased the possibility of over-involvement and subjectivity. the focus group meetings were conducted, transcribed, coded, and analysed by the course facilitator (also the researcher), which could have increased the possibility of subjectivity in the interpretation of results, despite several measures taken to prevent this. because the participants were not a homogeneous group (in terms of qualifications, literacy levels, prior knowledge, age, and language proficiency), it is possible that the pace of training was too fast for some, while appropriate for others. these factors also impacted on the completion of questionnaires and portfolio assignments. as the workshop material was prepared mainly in english, the participants were required to transfer their knowledge to the lolt, which hampered optimal learning. more examples are required in the lolt. district facilitators who are proficient in the lolt need to become more actively involved in the preparation of the workshop material, and need to be trained as co-trainers to bridge the language divide. it is also possible that less information provided in the workshops would have allowed more time for review, which could have increased the effectiveness of the training. as research informs practice, the limitations of the research also provide indicators for practice. indicators for the practices of slts • since it is not practically possible for a single slt to effectively support every teacher in an entire district, it is essential for slts and district facilitators to collaborate. district facilitators are required by the gde to support teachers in the implementation of the curriculum. they are responsible for the daily support of teachers and therefore need to be supported in their efforts to provide ongoing in-service training in literacy-related skills. • in a consultative and collaborative capacity, the slt can provide advice and support with cpd activities related to listening and language facilitation on an ongoing basis. in a collaborative model of support slts need to provide staff development activities to increase theoretical content knowledge and skills (king, strachan, tucker, duwyn, desserud, & shillington, 2009) as basis for pedagogical content knowledge. in turn, district facilitators are often proficient in the lolt and can contribute to the support process by using code switching to bridge the current language divide in workshops for teachers where slts are from a different language background. such a collaborative support programme needs to be developed as action research (burton & bartlett, 2005; onwuegbuzie & dickinson, 2007) as it will have to be adjusted over time to accommodate various topics and be tailor-made for specific contexts. • a once-off workshop by itself may be useful to introduce new ideas, but its effect is temporary (massel and goertz in roberts, 2002). a ‘cluster model’ of support as an alternative to large-group support may be more effective. the results of this study indicated that the participants preferred group learning and discussing issues and experiences in small groups while sitting around a table. group learning (cluster model) may be a more suitable support model for these particular contexts (killen, 2007). in an attempt to establish a balance between quantity and quality in training, the questions that need to be answered are whether cluster support contributes significantly more to the competence of teachers than large-group workshops and whether it warrants the costs. the advantages and disadvantages of such a cluster model (where small groups will be trained in short sessions over an extended period) as opposed to ‘once-off’ large-group training should be investigated. the effect of such small-group support could be determined by means of a case study design where both quantitative and qualitative methods are employed (roulstone, owen, & french, 2005). • reflective practices are inherent to the obe approach, but have not yet become familiar practice and need to be addressed in future programmes. dunst and trivette (2009) developed the participatory adult learning strategy (pals) which included ‘trainer-guided reflection’ to promote child literacy, communication and language learning practices to parents and slts, which could be further explored for use with teachers. continual reflection on their practices, together with practical experiences, will provide the basis for more effective professional growth for teachers and therapists. • the teachers in this study expressed a need for the slt (course facilitator) to observe their teaching and to provide individual feedback, which was beyond the scope of the research. in school-based support, the slts may want to support teachers through co-teaching and constructive feedback. however, effective collaboration requires that both parties understand their individual roles, and that slts take account of the educational environment. collaboration between slts and teachers cannot be taken for granted when these two professions are brought together as they stem from different disciplinary specialisation and knowledge bases. allen (in forbes, 2008: 153) is of the opinion that ‘collaboration with other professionals is a complex knot of relationships which has to be learned and worked at. it cannot be assumed that by issuing an enjoinder to collaborate, and by placing people together, that the outcomes will be positive.’ it is necessary to identify each discipline’s individual knowledge base and approaches, as well as the new knowledge, skills and approaches required to work together in supporting young learners in south african classrooms. with literacy and numeracy as central focus, the unique contribution of each profession needs to be determined in order to facilitate collaboration in schools. forbes (2008: 141) based a similar line of enquiry on the ‘analytic modes of knowledge’ described by gibbon, limoges, nowotny, schwartzman, scott, & trow (1994), which appears potentially useful as a starting point. however, more contextually relevant information is required for the south african context. conclusion the results showed that the provision of cpd activities regarding language learning can be effective as the majority of the participants implemented the strategies in class and valued the new skills acquired. teachers, as well as learners, reportedly enjoyed the strategies and learnt from them. in view of the relationship between language and literacy, it is imperative that teachers and slts work as a team towards a common goal of supporting learners in learning. as team members they need to show mutual respect and show an ability to work towards similar outcomes (o’toole & kirkpatrick, 2007). the results show that the support of teachers is beneficial for both teaching and learning, and that slts have an important role to play in the process. acknowledgement. the authors wish to acknowledge the support of the shuttleworth foundation for supporting the fieldwork. references asha (2001). roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists with respect to reading and writing in children and adolescents (guidelines). rockville, md: asha. burton, d., & bartlett, s. (2005). practitioner research for teachers. london: paul chapman publishing. coenders, f., terlouw, c., & dijkstra, s. (2008). assessing teachers’ beliefs to facilitate the transition to a new chemistry curriculum: what do teachers want? journal of science teacher education, 19(4), 317-335. department of education (2001). white paper 6: special needs education – building an inclusive education and training system. retrieved from http://www.polity.org.za/govdocs/white_papers/educ6.html. department of education (2002). revised national curriculum statement for schools: grades r 9. pretoria: department of education. department of education (2008). national strategy on screening, identification, assessment and support. pretoria: department of education. dunst, c.j., & trivette, c.m. (2009). let’s be pals: an evidence-based approach to professional development. infants and young children, 22(3), 164-176. forbes, j. (2008). knowledge transformations: examining the knowledge needed in teacher and speech and language therapist co-work. educational review, 60(2), 141-154. gibbon, m., limoges, c., nowotny, h., schwartzman, s., scott, p., & trow, m. (1994). the new production of knowledge. london: sage publications. gilmore, j., & vance, m. (2007). teacher ratings of children’s listening difficulties child language teaching and therapy, 23(2), 133-156. girolametto, l., weitzman, e., lefebvre, p., & greenberg, j. (2007). the effects of in-service education to promote emergent literacy in child care centres: a feasibility study. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 38, 72-83. johnson, k.l., & roseman, b.a. (2003). the source for phonological awareness. east moline, il: lingui systems. justice, l.m., & ezell, h. (2002). use of storybook reading to increase print awareness in at-risk children. american journal of speech-language pathology, 11, 17-29. justice, l.m., & ezell, h.k. (2001). written language awareness in preschool children from low income households: a descriptive analysis. communication disorders quarterly, 22, 123-134. justice, l.m., & kaderavek, j.n. (2004). embedded-explicit emergent literacy intervention: background and description of approach. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 35, 201-212. justice, l.m., skibbe, l., & ezell, h. (2006). using print referencing to promote written language awareness. in t.a. ukrainetz (ed.), contextualized language intervention: scaffolding pre k-12 literacy achievement (pp. 389-428). eau claire, wi: thinking publications. killen, r. (2007). teaching strategies for outcomes-based education (2nd ed.). cape town: juta. king, g., strachan, d., tucker, m., duwyn, b., desserud, s., & shillington, m. (2009). the application of a trans-disciplinary model for early intervention services. infants and young children, 22(3), 211-223. leedy, p.d., & ormrod, j.e. (2010). practical research (9th ed.). boston: pearson. lessing, a., & de wit, m.w. (2008, 30 september-1 october). do teachers know what the essential literacy skills are? paper presented at the laying solid foundations for learning meeting, makopane, limpopo. locke, a., ginsborg, j., & peers, i. (2002). development and disadvantage: implications for the early years and beyond. international journal of language and communication disorders, 37, 3-15. mcmillan, j.h., & schumacher, s. (2010). research in education: evidence-based inquiry. boston: pearson. miller, a., & watts, p. (1990). planning and managing effective professional development. harlow, essex, uk: longman. ministry of education (2001). national plan for higher education. pretoria: department of education. motshekga, a. (2010, 6 july 2010). statement by the minister of basic education, mrs angie motshekga, mp, on the progress of the review of national curriculum statement. retrieved on 17 august 2011 from http://www.education.gov.za. nancollis, a., lawrie, b.a., & dodd, b. (2005). phonological awareness intervention and the acquisition of literacy skills in children from deprived social backgrounds. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 36, 325-335. o’connor, j., & geiger, m. (2009). challenges facing primary school educators of english second (or other) language learners in the western cape. south african journal of education, 29, 253-269. o’toole, c., & kirkpatrick, v. (2007). building collaboration between professionals in health and education through interdisciplinary training. child language teaching and therapy, 23(3), 325-352. onwuegbuzie, a.j., & dickinson, w.b. (2007). mixed methods research and action research: a framework for the development of pre-service and in-service teachers. academic exchange. retrieved on 20 december 2008 from http://asstudents.unco.edu/students/ae-extra/2007/6/indxmain.html. owens, r.e. (2004). language disorders: a functional approach to assessment. boston, ma: allyn & bacon. paul, r. (2007). language disorders: from infancy through to adolescence (3rd ed.). st. louis, mo: mosby. rembe, s.w. (2005). the politics of transformation in south africa: an evaluation of education policies and their implementation with particular reference to the eastern cape province. grahamstown: rhodes university. roberts, j. (2002). district development: the new hope for educational reform. johannesburg: joint education trust. roulstone, s., owen, r., & french, l. (2005). speech and language therapy and the knowles edge standards fund project: an evaluation of the service provided to a cluster of primary schools. british journal of special education, 32(2), 78-85. stringer, e.t. (2007). action research (3rd ed.). los angeles, ca: sage. thomas muir scientific software development (2003-2004). atlas-ti: the knowledge workbench v5.0. berlin, germany: thomas muir scientific software development. wium, a.m. (2010). the development of a support programme for foundation phase teachers to facilitate listening and language for numeracy. pretoria: university of pretoria. wium, a.m., louw, b., & eloff, i. (2010). speech-language therapists supporting foundation phase teachers with literacy and numeracy in a rural and township context. south african journal of communication disorders, 57(1), 14-22. table i. the four language systems that children have to acquire aural system (language by ear) oral system (language by mouth) print system (language by eye) written system (language by hand) receptive language heard words expressive language spoken words receptive language printed words expressive language written words fig. 1. the link between language and literacy development. fig. 2. the relationship between listening, language, literacy and numeracy. fig. 3. the role of enjoyment in the programme. table ii. summary of the results obtained in the outcomes component area assessed results positive implementation of strategies 70% (n=125) positive benefits of the programme: learners 94% (n=132) participants 96% (n=137) enjoyment 95% (n=19) appendix a. workshop to facilitate language the training component of the cpd programme addressed the following topics: • what is language? as introduction to the cpd programme an explanation of language and why it is important was provided. there was a brief discussion on the aspects and the elements of language, and how it can be facilitated through the use of a variety of relevant activities and strategies (owens, 2004). • a balanced approach to facilitating reading and writing. current evidence (justice & kaderavek, 2004) regarding the acquisition of literacy skills suggests a balance of both contextualised and decontextualised (discrete) skill intervention as best practice. this specific programme supported a ‘balanced approach’ to the facilitation of literacy (justice & kaderavek, 2004), which creates opportunities to develop an understanding of the language and then uses this understanding as the basis to teach discrete skills within a phonics-oriented, code-based approach (justice, et al., 2006). such a balanced approach to literacy encompasses both the ‘top-down’ (whole language) and ‘bottom-up’ (phonetic) approaches and is most appropriate in the foundation phase where the focus is on facilitating literacy. the teachers were shown how language develops along a continuum, from oral language learnt in the home through concrete operations, to the decontextualised language required for written language used in school. in order to facilitate language and literacy skills teachers need to be aware of a balanced approach to facilitating reading and writing, and how to use central themes to facilitate the four language systems (listening, speaking, reading and writing) in an integrated manner. • the use of a theme in integrating the four language systems: the training component of the cpd programme made use of group activities where the teachers planned the use of songs, rhymes, and craft activities, together with listening, reading and writing exercises within a central theme. such an integrated thematic approach (ita) created several language-rich experiences and allowed the learners to not only develop the vocabulary related to a specific topic, but to integrate skills across the curriculum (department of education, 2002). 62 community work project in gazankulu: a community-based training experience melissa a. bortz, barbara schoub department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, south africa judy mckenzie rehabilitation unit, tintswalo hospital, acornhoek, gazankulu abstract speech pathology and audiology students at the university of the witwatersrand participated in afield trip to learn about rural community work. in collaboration with rehabilitation workers at tintswalo hospital, gazankulu, projects in pre-school language stimulation, aphasia assessment and intervention, and hearing screening were undertaken. projects adhered to community work principles. these were successful in terms of both providing a service to the community and teaching students principles and practice of community work. opsomming spraakheelkunde en audiologie studente van die universiteit van die witwatersrand het aan 'n plattelandsgemeenskapswerkprojek, as deel van hul onderrig, deelgeneem. projekte in voorskoolse taalstimulasie, afasie-evaluasie en terapie engehoorsifting, is in samewerking met die rehabilitasie-werkers van tintswalo hospitaal in gazankulu onderneem. die projekte het aan die beginsels vangemeenskapswerk voldoen. hierdie projekte was suksesvol, beide in terme van dienslewering aan die gemeenskap en die opleiding van die studente in die beginsels en praktyk van gemeenskapwerk. the discipline of epidemiology has been concerned with prevention of disease and is used to study relationships between people, their diseases and the agents that cause, prevent or cure their disease in the environment (kibbel & wagstaff, 1991). when considering prevention of communication disorders from an epidemiological perspective, prevention usually occurs at three levels, primary, secondary and tertiary (gerber, 1990). primary prevention is aimed at the promotion of good health by reducing the incidence of disease or where the occurrence of a communicative disorder can be eliminated, for example, by mass public education; secondary prevention is the early detection and treatment of a communication problem and aims at reducing the prevalence of disease while tertiary prevention is rehabilitation where traditionally speech and hearing therapists have focused their attention (butler, 1989; gerber, 1990). gerber (1990) does not deny that rehabilitation is necessary but has suggested that it would be positive "for humankind were we to have more primary and secondary prevention" (gerber, 1990, p. xiv). primary prevention can be instituted by means of primary health care which the who-unicef meeting (1978) cited by walt and vaughan (1981, p. 1) has declared is essential health care based on practical, scientifically sound and socially acceptable methods and technology, made universally accessible to individuals and families in the community through their full participation and at a cost that the community and country can afford to maintain at every stage of their development in the spirit of self-reliance and self determination. one of the methods currently used to institute primary health care is community work, which de vries (1984) believes is an effective means of promoting health through collective action to prevent disease. according to the calouste gulbenkian foundation (1973) community work is essentially concerned with affecting the course of social change through analysing social situations and forming relationships with different groups to bring about change (1973). j currently south africa's population is estimated at approximately 35 million people. in 1978 penn reported that although few incidence studies had been conducted in south africa those performed, indicate that 8-10% of the population have a communication difficulty. based upon these ifigures there could, currently, be as many as 3,5 million people with disorders of communication. drew (1982, p. 1) has contended that "there are vast sections of the population who do not receive even the most basic speech therapy and audiological care" and according to aron (1984), if one takes into account the number of speech, language and hearing therapists working, it appears that only approximately 138 000 patients are receiving any form of treatment. a possible reason for this is that speech and hearing therapy students are "not only drawn from the privileged sections of the population, but tend to operate within these sections" (drew, 1982, p. 1). in addition, adler (1979) has asserted that speech and hearing therapists tend to treat patients "in rather centralised geographic locations" (p. xiii). / the department of speech pathology and audiology at the university of the witwatersrand (wits) has been attempting to sasi.ha 1992 th south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) community work project in gazankulu deal comprehensively with the lack of speech and hearing personnel by the training of different tiers of workers who would nrovide services to broader spectrums of the community than currently occurs. (see model presented in figure 1). in addition, wits is striving to make the training of the four year degree in speech therapy and audiology more appropriate for the south african population. one of the methods that has been utilised, is community therapy so that "students can be made aware of the needs of different sectors of the community" (segal, 1982, p. 17). therefore, at present, community work principles are incorporated into all aspects of student training. one of the methods used to achieve this, is the use of regular field trips into the community to carry out speech, language and hearing based projects. the field trip that is to be described took place in the mhala district of gazankulu, a rural community with a population of approximately 200 000 people. most of the inhabitants live in villages of which there are 75, although there are also four towns. villagers have limited access to farming land and water needed for drinking and irrigation purposes. there is a high rate of unemployment and poverty, with people who are employed, being paid at the rate of between r2,00 and r5,00 per day (wits rural facility, 1991). because of limited employment opportunities, many people become migrant labourers, causing a breakdown in traditional family structure and culture. in addition, a large number of mozambican refugees have recently settled in this area, causing further strain on existing resources. medical facilities of this region consist of a 266 bed hospital (tintswalo), two health care centres, 10 permanent clinics and one mobile clinic. at tintswalo two occupational therapists (ots), a social worker, a part-time speech and hearing therapist, two community workers in speech and hearing, three assistants in occupational therapy, and two in physiotherapy, work together as a rehabilitation team. 63 in response to the need for making rehabilitation more accessible and affordable, the tintswalo rehabilitation team, in conjunction with the occupational therapy department of the university of the witwatersrand, is engaged in the process of training community rehabilitation workers at tintswalo hospital (unit for the development of rehabilitation strategies, 1992). the community rehabilitation workers are representatives selected from different villages in the mhala district and trained in basic principles of speech therapy, physiotherapy and occupational therapy and their application. the field trip was a joint project planned by the tintswalo rehabilitation team and the department of speech pathology at wits, and integrated with tintswalo's community rehabilitation worker training programme. the project was based upon methods used in speech and hearing such as hearing screening and community work principles, which "refer to various intervention approaches by a professional practitioner to help a community engage in conscious collective action in order to respond to social problems" (mitchell, 1987, p. 109). according to the calouste gulbenkian foundation (1973, p. 21), community work comprises certain characteristic steps which the field trip was designed to incorporate: 1) study the situation 2) establish rapport and a network of communication links with the community 3) gather information, analyse the relationship between community wants and needs and identify alternative ways of meeting needs 4) create some basis of organisation and of resources, work on the programme, modifying procedures and objectives as experience grows 5) review and develop progress to determine the next steps. in speech and hearing therapists: 4 tear university degree course (prevention, promotion, χ rehabilitation and research n concerning communication impairment) community workers in speech and hearing theradv: 2 year diploma course (prevention, promotion and rehabilitation of communication impairment) community-based rehabilitation workers: }"2 year situ rehabilitation training with general disability figure 1: proposed model for personnel involved in treatment of communication problems. (department of speech pathology and! audiology, university of the witwatersrand). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 melissa a. bortz, barbara schoub & judy mckenzie mitchell (1987) has reported that the final stage of a community work project is evaluation which is the process by which the participants in a project assess the extent to which the project has met its objectives. cooper and heenan (1980) believe that evaluation should be carried out on an ongoing basis while the project is in progress, and a concluding evaluation session should be held in which the overall project is evaluated in terms of the extent to which it has met the stated expectations and goals. the field trip aim the purpose of the field trip, for the wits students was to learn community work principles and experience conditions in rural areas. the aim of the field trip for the tintswalo rehabilitation team was to engage in speech and language projects they had identified as necessary but had not carried out due to lack of adequate personpower. for the community the purpose of the field trip was to receive language and hearing services. participants three staff members and 14 students from wits participated in the three selected projects for the field trip, together with members of the tintswalo rehabilitation team and community rehabilitation workers. all participants in the field trip were divided into the three selected projects discussed below. p l a n n i n g feasibility and organizational details were discussed at a planning.workshop held in gazankulu with members of staff from the tintswalo rehabilitation team, from the community rehabilitation worker training programme and from the department of speech pathology and audiology, wits. this took place a month before the field trip. three projects were selected to be undertaken by participants in the field trip. these were new projects selected for the field trip according to needs previously identified in the community by the tintswalo rehabilitation team. the selected field trip projects were: 1. a language stimulation project consisting of wits students and staff and a community worker in speech and hearing which aimed to educate childminders at local creches about the development of language and the importance of language stimulation and play. 2. a stroke rehabilitation project whose objectives were for wits staff and students to train student community rehabilitation workers about speech and language problems associated with stroke. a further objective was to devise relevant and appropriate assessment and management procedures for aphasic patients in this setting. 3. a hearing screening and education project aimed at identifying hearing problems by conducting hearing screening at three schools. in addition, this group would be used to provide information about hearing loss and management to teachers and members of a self help group that had deaf members. the month between the planning workshop and the field trip was used to organise such practical details as transport from wits to tintswalo, and in gazankulu; and accommodation for wits students and other participants in the language stimulation group. to the writers' knowledge this is one of the first occasions that a project of this nature, combining the fields of community work and speech and hearing has taken place, and there are no recognised evaluation procedures or standardised measures for projects of this nature. therefore, much of the work undertaken was exploratory, which the reader should bear in mind, for the remainder of the article. implementation the duration of the field trip was four days with projects being carried out for three days and the fourth day being used for each group to discuss and evaluate their project.for a detailed timetable of the trip, see appendix. 1. language stimulation project the aim of this project was for wits staff and students and members of the tintswalo rehabilitation team to provide information about language development, stimulation and play to childminders belonging to a support group in gazankulu. background information. the tintswalo rehabilitation team coordinates a support programme for childminders at local creches and has organised an association called hlangavani creche teachers support group whose aim is: 1) to provide support and training for childminders in the region, through the running of workshops by training associations such as the montessori society of child centered education. childminders are generally untrained and often work on a voluntary basis. 2) to assist with fundraising for creches in this region. reynolds (1989) has contended that in societies such as the one being described, adults tend not to play with their children as play is regarded as being undignified. in addition, it has been noted by members of the tintswalo rehabilitation team that parents tend to perceive feeding and day-care rather than preschool education, to be the primary functions of creche. members of the tintswalo rehabilitation team, based on their professional knowledge, considered that the time the children spent at creche could be maximally utilised by engaging in such activities as play. childminders had also agreed to this when receiving information regarding the importance of play, at regular interactions between them and members of the tintswalo rehabilitation team. thus, the need for a programme, in the form of a workshop, dealing with language stimulation and play had been identified by childminders and the tintswalo rehabilitation team. j the childminders were notified about the proposed .workshop by one of the ots from the tintswalo rehabilitation team, who organised accommodation for them. eight 'childminders attended the workshop which was facilitated by six wits students, a staff member and a community worker in speech and hearing. the aim of the facilitators was to increase the childminders' motivation to learn and act on this learning, as suggested by brookfield (1986). in order to obtain maximal benefit and participation, members were divided into two equal groups from the second day of the programme. the same format was adopted for both groups. location. the workshops were held at: 1. pfukani creche which has an enrolment of 30 children and primarily caters for the children of tintswalo hospital staff. a number of disabled children also attend this creche. 2. hluvukani creche which is a community-creche catering for 250 children, mostly refugees. this creche is assisted by operation hunger. the programme was conducted in english at pfukani creche as all participants could communicate in english. at hluvukani creche, however, as two caregivers did not understand english, all input was translated by a member of the group who was fluent in both english and tsonga. the comthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) community work project in gazankulu 65 munity worker in speech and hearing, although fluent in both languages, was not used as the translator, as her role in this group was that of facilitator. the programme. according to twelvetrees (1986, p. 21) certain basic methods are followed in community work, that is, "to study the situation, establish rapport and gather information" and in accordance with this, the functioning of the two creches and the interaction of childminders with the children were observed by the facilitators on the first day. this observation confirmed the need, previously identified, to provide childminders with information about language stimulation. it was apparent that childminders were not allowing children to play, and that they were providing much information at inappropriately high levels: eg. teaching three to six year old shangaan speakers approximately 15 new english words in the space of 10 minutes, by having them imitate the childminder. this was particularly true at pfukani creche. based on the information obtained, the facilitators prepared an educational programme consisting of talks and activities such as role play and poster presentations. information was to be provided on language and its development, language stimulation, the importance of play and how to incorporate this into a creche programme. this programme was implemented on the second and third day of the field trip with techniques such as role-playing, modelling and problem solving exercises being used to demonstrate principles and methods of the programme, rather than lecturing to childminders on the specific topics. the example noted at the observation, of the childminders attempting to teach english vocabulary by imitation, was used to demonstrate the difficulty of learning a language in this way, by the facilitators using the same ineffective methods of teaching a foreign language, hebrew, to the childminders.an example of one of the subjects addressed in the workshop was effective story telling. this was demonstrated through role-play followed by a discussion with the facilitators and childminders on the principles of story telling. childminders were then asked to role-play telling a story to the workshop. finally, the childminders were given the opportunity of telling the story to children in the creches. other similar role-plays and discussions were held over the two days to demonstrate other principles of language stimulation and play such as teaching body parts and singing. posters were also used to explain the sequence of language development. evaluation evaluation was carried out on an ongoing basis in accordance with the evaluation procedures, previously described by cooper and heenan (1980). in the ongoing evaluation it was noted by the facilitators that the childminders participated enthusiastically in all activities and had acquired new and more effective skills in stimulating language development, in such activities as teaching vocabulary or songs. when teaching vocabulary childminders attempted to introduce only one or two new words into an activity. they would explain the word, for example, a particular object, by showing it to the children, describing it, enumerating its uses and allowing the children to play with it. in addition, the childminders displayed an ability to generate and expand on what they had been taught, for example by the incorporation of songs that the children knew, into a story. this resulted in the exposure of the children to more varied activities and input. in the formal evaluation session held at the end of the third day, the childminders described the workshop positively and said that it had benefitted them. examples provided of what they had learned were that they had not realised that children learn through play or that language learning is a gradual procedure. the success of the project, which was manifest both in terms of the increase in skills and the positive evaluation of participants can be seen as consistent with the principles of community work, that is language stimulation had been identified as a need, and therefore the childminders were motivated to carry out the programme as it served their needs and their potential for becoming self-sufficient, as advanced by segal (1982). the students also evaluated the workshop positively. the students at hluvakani creche found that the translation of the proceedings from english to tsonga slowed down the process of giving information. however, they reported that this was extremely valuable in terms of exposing them to the inherent problem of not always speaking the same language as the client, so common in south africa. the community worker in speech and hearing commented on the positive changes she had seen taking place in the childminders interaction with their pupils. her feedback was of particular value as she had experienced the childminders' previous lack of knowledge regarding language stimulation and play, while during the workshop she was able to view a constructive change in this. she expressed concern that these skills should be maintained and therefore implemented long term evaluation of this project by involving the tintswalo rehabilitation team and the hlangvani creche teachers support group. 2. the stroke rehabilitation project the aim of the project was to assist community rehabilitation workers in the practical aspects of identifying and assessing aphasia and planning suitable intervention procedures. formal standardised aphasia tests for this population are not available, therefore a further aim was to develop basic identification and assessment procedures, suitable for use by the community rehabilitation workers. participants. pairs, consisting of a wits student and a community rehabilitation worker visited individual clients in the community rehabilitation workers' villages. a wits staff member and a tintswalo rehabilitation team member provided transport for the pairs and consulted with each pair when necessary to assist in evaluation. the programme. at the planning workshop, the following timetable was proposed. on the first day stroke patients who had previously been referred to members of the tintswalo rehabilitation team or community rehabilitation workers, were to be observed in their homes. on the basis of these observations an aphasia evaluation and home therapy programme was to be devised, on the second day. the assessment was to be carried out with patients and its effectiveness assessed on the third day, and finally, on the fourth day, the home therapy programme was to be initiated and its success evaluated. due to the considerable distances between villages however it was not practical to keep to this proposed timetable. this difficulty had not been foreseen initially. in addition, the implementation of the therapy programme on day four could not be carried out as many of the previously identified stroke patients did not present with aphasia, or were too frail and ill to receive therapy. the programme for day one was implemented as planned. the observations of the stroke patients in their homes revealed that many of the patients were not aphasic. it was also realised that it took considerably longer than had been anticipated to transport pairs to their locations, in some instances up to two hours per pair. one of the reasons was that no maps or street names are available for the different villages and time was the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 melissa a. bortz, barbara schoub & judy mckenzie spent in asking directions from local residents. on day two an aphasia assessment was devised for those stroke patients who appeared to present with aphasia. broad areas were to be examined, for example, how to test receptive and expressive language, using objects and stimuli available in the patients' homes. this was successfully implemented on the third day. also on this day, where appropriate, suggestions were made to families on how to communicate more effectively with their aphasic family member. at the request of the community rehabilitation workers the remainder of the day was used to assist them with other patients they had identified with communication problems such as mentally retarded or dysarthric children. implications it was invaluable for wits students to see patients in their home environments as this helped them to understand the overwhelming physical difficulties handicapped patients in rural areas have to contend with, eg. the uneven and eroded terrain that patients have to negotiate when going to the toilet. in addition, they were able to observe the close involvement of the extended family and neighbours in caring for the patient. this highlighted the need for the community rehabilitation worker to provide the family and community with education about stroke and management of the communication problems of the patient. it was particularly valuable for wits staff and students, as members of a training institution, to experience the importance of community rehabilitation workers. this was evidenced in terms of their ability to relate to the patient and the family. it was clear that the shared community origins were most valuable in that the community rehabilitation workers understood the language, local customs and the needs of their community, which endorses segal's observations (segal, 1982). furthermore, the presence of community rehabilitation workers in the villages, has made integrated assessment and management programmes across disciplines, available in the homes of patients otherwise deprived of access to rehabilitation, which is available only in places far from their homes. as mentioned in an earlier section, community rehabilitation workers receive training in basic assessment and treatment principles of physiotherapy, occupational therapy and speech therapy. the benefit of this broad training gives them an integrated perspective of aphasia in relation to the patient's other disabilities, and helps them to treat patients holistically. this is a perspective which is often difficult for members of different paramedical professions to appreciate. this project was successful too, in that the wits students imparted specialist skills to the community rehabilitation workers to allow them to provide services to their own communities. according to wagstaff (1984, p. 11) "primary health care advocates ... team work and the responsible delegation of tasks for which necessary training has been given". from the experience of teaching students it is clear that there is a common misconception that community work must of necessity be carried out within groups. this project helped students to realise that community work can be carried out on a one to one basis. students also learned about the need to be flexible when undertaking community work, as they observed how programmes had to be modified due to circumstances that could not be predicted, for example, the small number of patients who had aphasia, necessitated a change to the programme. the assessment procedure was successful in identifying the presence or absence of aphasia. however, as few of the patients exhibited aphasia, it was not possible to demonstrate to the community rehabilitation workers the full evaluation of aphasic symptoms, such as perseveration or word finding difficulties. furthermore, due to the fact that there were so few aphasics, and that these were mostly elderly and many years post stroke, intervention procedures could not be demonstrated. 3. hearing screening and education project background information. this project took place in the village of croquet lawn following an incidence study undertaken by the tintswalo rehabilitation team in 1990, in which numerous patients with hearing loss were identified. in addition two deaf members belong to a self-help group that undertakes sewing and agriculture, with the purpose of obtaining an income which has been established in this area. it should be noted that self-help is a strategy that most typifies community work and aims'to assist people in learning how to deal with their problems according to mitchell (1987). aims. the aims for this project included hearing screening of children in the village pre-primary and primary school and diagnostic testing of pupils with known hearing losses in the high school. in addition educational talks providing information to teachers on such topics as hearing loss, basic ear hygiene and management of the hearing impaired child in the classroom were held. a final aim of this project was a talk to members of the self-help group to discuss deafness, causes of hearing loss and communication with the hearing impaired. this was conducted in oral and sign language. participants. a staff member and four students from wits; a community worker in speech and hearing; and a community rehabilitation worker from croquet lawn were to administer this project. the programme. hearing screening of the pre-primary school took place on the first day of the project while the primary school was screened on the second day. otoscopic examinations preceded the screening. as there was no electricity, a liverpool screening audiometer and peters audiometers were used. screening levels were 50db at 500 and 1000hz and 30db at 2000 and 4000hz on the liverpool audiometer due to high levels of ambient noise and 35db at 500, 1000, 2000 and 4000hz on the peters audiometer. in addition on the second day, diagnostic audiometry was administered to pupils at the high school who had previously been identified as having hearing problems. thorough pure tone testing as suggested by green (1985) was attempted but was not possible due to the fact that portable audiometers were used and no sound proof booths were available. educational talks were delivered to teachers and members of the self-help group on the third day of the project, on the topics mentioned above. both talks incorporated a discussion on the role of jthe community rehabilitation workers as regards identification, monitoring and referral of patients with hearing problems in the community. results. all the aims of this project were achieved. otoscopic examinations revealed a high incidence of impacted wax. referrals for removal of this were made to the local clinic or to the community worker in speech and hearing, who has a great deal of experience with wax removal. children with suspected ear infections were referred to the local clinic for treatment and children who failed the hearing screening, where there was no evidence of impacted wax or ear'infection, were referred to tintswalo hospital for further diagnostic testing. evaluation. this project was successful in that the following was achieved: hearing screening was successful in identifying children who had ear problems arid needed treatment or referral. a positive outcome was that the students learned to apply their screening skills,in a rural community and they reported bethe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) community work project in gazankulu 67 coming more proficient in otoscopic examination. this project was also successful in emphasising the importance of the community rehabilitation worker and tintswalo rehabilitation team in providing follow up for identified hearing problems. the importance of being appropriately prepared and of identifying community resources prior to embarking on such a project, was highlighted by participants in this project. for example, it was important to know before the field trip that there was no electricity in the pre-primary and primary schools, and that therefore it would be necessary to use batteryoperated audiometers. in addition, participants learned how to adapt basic procedures for the situation, for example, diagnostic testing with a portable audiometer. summary and conclusions when evaluating the results of the field trip it is important to remember that it lasted four days. within that time, change was seen, but it is not yet clear whether there can be any long term effects in such a short time. results and conclusions should thus be seen in the light of this. the specific objectives of the three projects were achieved with the exception of implementing aphasia assessment and intervention. this was due to factors that could not have been anticipated such as the fact that not all stroke patients had aphasia. during the exposure to community work, participants learned the importance of being flexible and of being able to adapt to existing conditions. the successful working relationships between speech and hearing therapists, community workers in speech and hearing, and community rehabilitation workers, as well as occupational therapists, was proved to be particularly appropriate for the context of the field trip. in addition, results of the field trip indicated that a coordinated team such as the one used, provided an integrated and effective community service. a large part of the success of this service can be attributed to the fact that follow up of referrals was made possible by the existing infrastructure of the tintswalo rehabilitation teams and community rehabilitation workers. it would have been irresponsible to undertake community work projects without providing the necessary follow up services. as regards the aims of the various participants of the field trip, the tintswalo rehabilitation team considered the field trip successful as the projects were planned according to their identified needs and provided the personpower to undertake them. the community benefitted in that they received necessary language and hearing services.the wits students and staff, on the other hand gained an insight and increased proficiency for community work from this field trip. much of the value was derived from the reality of the aims and the work done. this trip did not merely provide an opportunity for 'rural tourism'. finally, this project achieved, to a certain extent, an objective of community work which is to enable the communities to acquire the skills and confidence to tackle problems which are necessary to ensure thatconcrete changes are brought about in the community (twelvetrees, 1986). acknowledgements the writers gratefully acknowledge the assistance of ms. m. schneider for participating in this project under laborious circumstances. the assistance of the students is also warmly appreciated. references adler, s. (1979). poverty children and their language: implications for teaching and treating. new york: grune and stratton. aron, m. a. (1984). introduction. proceedings of conference on communitg work in speech and hearing therapg. johannesburg: university of the witwatersrand. brookfield, s. d. (1986). understanding and facilitating adult learning. milton keynes: oxford university press. butler, j. (1989). child health surveillance in primary care: a critical review. london: her majesty's stationery office. the calouste gullbenkian foundation. (1973). current issues in communitg work. a study by the communitg workgroup. london: routledge & kegan paul. cooper, s., & heenan, c. (1980). preparing, designing and leading workshops: a humanistic approach. boston: cbi publishing, de vries, g. (1984). community work in health. proceedings of conference on communitg work in speech and hearing therapg. johannesburg: university of the witwatersrand. drew, μ. e. (1982). editorial. south african speech and hearing association newsletter, 250, 1-6. gerber, s. e. (1990). prevention: the etiology of communicative disorders in children. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice hall. green, d. s. (1985). pure tone air-conduction testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). (pp. 98-109).baltimore: williams and wilkins. kibel, m. a. & wagstaff, l. a. (1991). child health for all: a manual far southern africa. cape town: oxford university press. mitchell, w. (1987). social work with communities. in b. w. mckendrick (ed.), introduction to social work in south africa. pinetown: owen burgess. penn, c. (1978). speech pathology and audiology in south africa past, present and future perspectives. in l. w. lanham & k.p. prinsloo (eds.), language and communication studies in south africa: current issues and directions in research and inquiry. cape town: oxford university press. reynolds, p. (1989). childhood in crossroads: cognition and society in south africa. claremont: david phillip. segal, d. (1982). community-based therapy: some preliminary suggestions. south african speech and hearing association newsletter. 251. 15-18. twelvetrees, a. (1986). communitg work. london: macmillan education. unit for the development of rehabilitation strategies. (1992). report on the training of communitg rehabilitation worker students. unpublished document. tintswalo hospital, gazankulu. wagstaff, l. a. (1984). the need to work in the community. proceedings of conference on communitg work in speech and hearing therapy. johannesburg: university of the witwatersrand. walt, g. & vaughan, p. (1981). an introduction to the primary health care approach in developing countries. a review with selected annotated references. ross institute of tropical hygiene publication no 13. london. wits rural facility. (1991). introduction to the mhala region of the eastern transvaal. unpublished document. wits rural facility. address correspondence to ms. m. bortz, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, private bag 3, wits, 2050, south africa. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 melissa a. bortz, barbara schoub & judy mckenzie appendix 3 proposed programme for field trip to gazankulu, 1 5 september,! 992. projects 1. language stimulation 2. stroke rehabiliation 3. hearing screening and education day 1 observe at creches observe at stroke patients' homes screen hearing at pre-primary school day 2 implement programme devise aphasia evaluation and home therapy programme screen hearing at primary school. diagnostic audiometry at high school. day 3 implement programme and evaluate implement assessment, evaluate and counsel families educational talks to teachers and self-help group. day 4 institute home therapy programme evaluation of field trip by prticipants the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the effect of intracranial pressure on the performance of an aphasic subject: a case history michelle shapiro b . a . ( s p . & h . t h e r a p y ) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) speech and hearing therapy department. baragwanath hospital. summary a battery of tests involving the language modalities of speech, comprehension, reading and writing were administered to an aphasic subject who presented with a predictable fluctuation in intracranial pressure. results revealed a consistency in performance in the two situations, across all modalities. the subject's performance was found to be of a concrete nature, demonstrating a cognitive deficit concomitant with his linguistic impairment. opsomming .'n battery toetse wat toetsing van die taalmodaliteite van spraak, begrip, lees en skryf insluit is uitgevoer op 'n afasiepatient wat gepresenteer het met 'n vooispelbare fluktuasie van intrakraniale druk. resultate het konstantheid van gedrag in die twee situasies t.o.v. alle modaliteite aangetoon. die pasient se gedrag was konkreet van aard, wat dui op die kognitiewe ontoereikendheid wat met 'n linguistiese afwyking gepaardgaan. the performance of aphasics has been studied under the influence of alerting and tranquillizing drugs, fatigue induced by exercise, relaxation training, influence of motivating instructions and scheduling effects.18 one variable that has been given little consideration as to its potential influence on measurement of aphasic patients' communicative skills is intracranial pressure. the present study was concerned with the effects of changes in intracranial pressure on an aphasic subject's expressive and receptive language, reading and writing skills. schuell, jenkins, and jiminez-pabon2 2 consider aphasia to be a general deficit that crosses all language modalities and which may or may not be complicated by other sequelae of brain damage. marie and goldstein1 6 believe that the principal form of the mental process is "symbolic activity", put into operation as "abstract" schemes, and that every disease of the brain is manifested, not so much by loss of the ability to carry out specialized processes as by a depression of this symbolic function or abstract orientation, (p. 19). having stated these definitions it is opportune to pose two questions as yet unresolved in the literature. is the aphasic patient impaired in his thinking because of a deficit in the language, or is he impaired in language because of a "primary, adventitiously acquired" deficit in thinking?1 0 a second question posed by eisenson 1 0 is particularly relevant to this study. compared with his premorbid potential, is the aphasic an intellectually impaired person and so, at best, one who will continue to be linguistically impaired in that he will not recover fully his capacities for dealing with high-level abstract propositions?10 (p. 1224). throughout the literature a general viewpoint emerges suggesting that the higher mental functions are formed in the process of ontogenesis, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 michelle shapiro passing through several successive stages. 1 1 ' 1 2 · 1 6 piaget 1 2 does not emphasize individual differences in intelligence, but has evolved a description of the general form of thought. his theories depict the optimum capabilities of thought at a given period in development. at successive stages in their development, the structure of the higher mental functions does not remain constant, but performs the same task by means of different, regularly interchanging systems of connections.1 6 therefore, it can be concluded that reading, writing, receptive and expressive speech have different psychological compositions at different stages in development. m e t h o d o l o g y a i m the primary aim of the study was to determine the effect of intracranial pressure changes on the communicative skills (reading, writing, receptive and expressive language) of an aphasic subject. the performance of the subject with variation in intracranial pressure (i.c.p.) was attained by testing the patient in the morning; that is, when the pressure had been built up after approximately eight hours sleep. in the afternoon the lower pressure recordings were made; that is, when the pressure had dropped after the day's activities in the upright position. if no change is evident in the performance of the subject at high and low intracranial pressure, the writer will attempt to determine the possible contribution of a cognitive deficit to his linguistic impairment. it was hypothesized that the communicative skills will be improved when the intracranial pressure is at a higher level; and the converse; that is, that there is a detectable decline in performance when the pressure is lower. s u b j e c t the subject used in this study was an english-speaking male, aged forty-nine years, who sustained an injury to his head when he fell six years before. a linear tempero-parieto-occipital fracture of the left hemisphere and a contrecoup lesion of the right frontal lobe was noted. a subdural haemorrhage developed on the left side. within a week his head swelled and due to the excessively high i.c.p. he had to undergo an emergency, life-saving operation in which a haemotoma was removed. a bone flap was removed to provide adequate decompression. however, the decompression site became progressively more swollen, and a communicating hydrocephalus developed. as a result the i.c.p. rose to a danger level. the subject became semi-comatose, and in order to reduce this pressure, surgery was performed in which a ventriculo-atrial shunt was inserted. however, the opposite effect now occurred. pressure manometry revealed a damped (low) i.c.p. system (5 ml h 2 0 compared with the normal 130 ml h 2 0 ) with minimal rises in' pressure with coughing, valsalva the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) i n t r a c r a n i a l pressure and aphasia 83 manoeuvre, speaking and posture. to ensure adequate circulation of blood through the brain a higher i.c.p. was required. two further operations were performed. firstly, a stellon prosthesis was sutured to the bone and periosteum, thus separating the i.c.p. from the barometric (atmospheric) pressure and hence creating a closed system. in the second operation, an "on-off" switch to the ventriculo-atrial shunt and an i.c.p. transducer were inserted. pressure recordings were made with the valve switched " o n " and "off'. in order to determine the relationship between the subject's i.c.p. and level of brain functioning, the relationship between the i.c.p. and fluid volume was established. a stress curve was used to act as a standard for the interpretation. this is represented in figure 1. in a normal subject, the cerebo-spinal fluid (c.s.f.) pressure is set to a steady level by externally induced forces, mainly from the respiration and less so from the heart. to obtain figure 1, controlled amounts of c.s.f. were first removed and then ringer lactate solution added, with a pause at each station to allow the newly induced level to adjust spontaneously to a steady state, thus introducing three dimensional parameters of volume, pressure and 20 18 16 14 12 volume/ml 10 8 2 2ml units ringer lactate ^ key: = functioning before shunt/plale was inserted = present status (with plate) = normal functioning = hydrocephalic patient (the status of the subject before the valve was inserted). figure 1. a diagramatic representation of the subject's i.c.p. functioning with an increase in cerebro-spinal fluid volume. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 84 michelle shapiro time.2· 3 2 ml of fluid is injected at a time by means of a lumbar puncture and the rise in pressure is monitored. as can be seen in a normal subject each time 2 ml is injected, the pressure rises. however when a certain pressure is reached through stress or some other deviation, any increase in pressure can prove to be dangerous. the human copes with this by what has been termed restoring forces that oppose the distortion and reduce the pressure. the restoration appears to depend essentially on the overall elasticity of the system. therefore, each stress curve. is likely to be individual specific and pathology specific. in the aphasic subject, by comparison, the shunt was originally overacting or overdraining and as a result the subject's opening pressure was about 5 ml h 2 0 . with the insertion of the plate the subject's opening pressure rose 5 ml h z o which is better than his previous one but below the normal opening pressure. originally with an increase of volume, the pressure rose but then fell back to its original level. after a few increases in volume a further increase in volume did not result in a pressure increase. the supine position facilitates the flow of blood to the brain. upon rising in the morning the i.c.p. of the subject is at its optimum; that is approximating normal levels. physiologically, this forms the basis for the improved functioning in the morning: the subject reported better performance in his speaking, reading, writing and comprehension. owing to the valve function the i.c.p. reduces as the day progresses and consequently his performance declines.1 in this study the writer attempted to assess the reported changes in the subject's higher mental functions; that is, expressive and receptive speech, reading and writing skills. tests the battery of tests administered to assess the possible performance changes consisted of standardized and non-standardized tests. the standardized tests chosen had either test — retest reliability or equivalent forms in order to permit unbiased repeated examination of the same patient during conditions of high and low i.c.p. the aphasic tests used which adhere to these criteria include the boston diagnostic aphasia examination, 1 4 the shortened version of the token test 6 and the reporter's test. 7 the neale reading analysis2 0 and picture story language t e s t 1 9 are also standardized tests. these were included in the battery, as reading and writing form part of/higher cortical functioning. the abovementioned tests can be quantitatively and qualitatively analysed. the tests compiled by p e n n 2 1 were also included in the battery to assess the subject's competence with respect to complex sentences. finally, expressive samples of speech were analysed according to crystal, fletcher and garman's 4 language assessment, remediation and screening procedure (larsp). the procedure was used to provide a description of the subject's syntactic output. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) i n t r a c r a n i a l pressure and aphasia 85 to confirm the trends which emerged from the aforementioned battery, the wechsler intelligence scalefor children — revised (wisc-r) was administered to assess the subject's verbal and non-verbal performances.2 4 testing procedure with the exception of the wisc, each test or equivalent form of the test was administered on two separate occasions, with a week's interval. an hour was scheduled for each session to reduce the possibility of the subject becoming fatigued during the assessment period. testing extended over ten days. one assessment was scheduled to be held in the morning at 8.30;.that is, at the time of higher i.c.p. the second assessment was scheduled for the afternoon at 3.30 the following week. order of scheduling, that is, morning first, afternoon s e c o n d ; and afternoon first, morning second, was randomly determined. (refer table i.) an attempt was made to present approximately half the tests first in the morning and the other half first in the afternoon. table i: the testing procedure name of test order of administration name of test morning (8,30 a.m.) higher i.c.p. afternoon (3,30 p.m.) lower i.c.p. \ boston (first i) 1st 2nd 5 boston (second i) 2nd 1st shortened version of the token test 2nd 1st reporter's test 1st 2nd complex sentences (comprehension and recall) 2nd 1st description of an orange/ banana 1st 2nd how to attach a plug/ change a tyre 2nd 1st description of a picture (from boston test) 1st 2nd neale reading analysis 1st 2nd picture story language test 2nd 1st die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 86 michelle shapiro results no marked difference in the subject's performance with higher and lower i.c.p. was evident from the battery of tests administered. (see table ii.) on the boston diagnostic aphasia examination, the z-score profiles of aphasia subscores revealed a picture of "conduction aphasia" but showed little variation between measurements at higher and lower i.c.p. on the shortened version of the token test, the subject performed better at times of lower i.c.p. on all parts of the test. penn's comprehension subtest 2 1 involved asking the subject questions about specific aspects of complex sentences presented orally. the subject displayed a consistent confusion of the deep relationship for each type of complex sentence as well as for sentences containing more than one embedding. the confusion of the deep relations was apparent at both the high and low i.c.p. performances, although the frequency of occurrence differed. twenty-seven sentences were administered at each sitting, with a corresponding seventy-two questions. the subject's comprehension performance was found to be better at higher i.c.p. the larsp profiles revealed consistent performance of the subject at high and low i.c.p. in terms of the length of the sample; mean sentence length; distribution of the structures on the clause, phrase and word levels; number of expansions; and error types. the results of the complex sentence-recall subtest were analysed qualitatively and indicated a better performance at low i.c.p. on the reporter's test, no difference was found in the performance of the subject at high and low i.c.p. results from the neale reading analysis revealed little change in performance at high and low i.c.p. on equivalent forms of the test. (form β and form c.) the subject performed equally poorly at high and low i.c.p. on the picture story language test. on the abstract-concrete scale the subject obtained a score of " 6 " which falls within level ii; that is, the "concrete-descriptive" level. the wisc-r was administered only once, in the afternoon, to ascertain the relationship between the subject's verbal and non-verbal performances with respect to an age equivalent. results revealed that he was functioning verbally on an 8 year 11 month level and non-verbally on an 11 year 10 month level. / discussion the results of this study did not support the hypothesis that there is an improvement in performance at the time of higher i.c.p. the subject's performance was consistent throughout the testing period regardless of the state of i.c.p. although an improvement in the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) intracranial pressure and aphasia t a b l e ii: summary of the subject's performances at higher and lower i.c.p. task administered high i.c.p. low i.c.p. the boston diagnostic aphasia examination + + the shortened version of the token test + complex sentences: comprehension + complex sentences: recall + description of an orange/banana + instructions: how to attach a plug/change a tyre + + reporter's test + + neale reading analysis + + picture story language test + + + improved performance deteriorated performance + consistent performance comprehension performance was found with higher i.c.p., the amount of change was not marked. due to the fact that the neurosurgeon confirmed a physiological basis for improved performance, and the fact that the subject reported improved performance with higher i.c.p., it was important on the basis of the results to investigate the possibility of another factor contributing to the consistency in the subject's performance throughout testing. it was for this reason that the writer attempted to determine if the subject presented with a cognitive deficit concomitant with his linguistic impairment. goldstein 1 3 states that there is a change in an aphasic's behaviour in the ability to employ an abstract attitude. the essence of change is in a tendency towards concretism.9 (p. 1224.) in piaget's theory, the concrete-operational period takes place in the normal child between the ages of seven and eleven years, whereas the formal operational period (which he considers to be the final period of intellectual development) begins at about twelve years of age and is consolidated during adolescence. 1 1 , 1 2 inhelder and piaget 1 5 state that both the syntax and the semantics of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 88 michelle shapiro language involve structures of classification, (p. 2.) as the subject performed poorly in both these areas, one would hypothesize that he experiences difficulty in "classification", and therefore, can attribute his poor performance to an underlying cognitive impairment. an analysis based on the fact that not all word classes are understood in the same w a y , 1 3 revealed that the subject experienced more difficulty understanding nouns than adjectives on the shortened version of the token test 6 at lower i.c.p. however, de renzi and vignolo8 assert that the nouns used in the token test, are, in fact, nouns which have the function of adjectives ("square" for example, identifies an attribute of the token, as does "red"). they forward a possible explanation for the difference in error count; namely, that shape is a more abstract concept than colour or size, and that geometrical conceptions and their linguistic equivalents are learned later than size and colour discrimination in childhood. this confirms the finding that the subject was operating at a concrete rather than an abstract level. the subject's poor performance at high and low i.c.p. on the complex sentence-comprehension subtest illustrated clearly his inability to understand the meaning of complicated logico-grammatical combinations which express certain abstract relationships. in analysing the six larsp profiles of expressive speech it was found that the subject only used three subordinate clauses in the samples, and no instances of more than one subordinate clause in a particular utterance was evident. davelaar5 cited bernstein who found a correlation between the amount of subordinate clauses that children use and the level of cognitive development. h e suggested that a greater number of subordinate clauses implied a more advanced level of cognitive development. the subject was unable to repeat sentences on both presentations of the complex sentence-recall subtest. luria and h u t t o n 1 7 postulate that the difficulty experienced in repeating as opposed to speaking, naming and understanding is due to the fact that repeating is an artificial task requiring certain abstractions that are not present in normal speech activity. they, therefore, assumed that the inability to repeat words or sentences is associated with an impairment of complex forms of abstract speech behaviour. results of the reporter's test revealed a breakdown in the subject's ability to provide all the necessary information for a third person to reproduce the examiner's performance. this could be attributed to an impairment in cognition. a contemporary linguistic view is that writing is a parallel or alternative form of language to speech, and that reading, like listening, involves a direct "decoding to meaning", or comprehension.2 3 on the neale reading analysis the subject obtained accuracy scores equivalent to a 7,1 and 8,6 year old respectively with high and low the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) i n t r a c r a n i a l pressure and aphasia 89 i c p., rate of reading scores of 6,11 and 7,2 years respectively and comprehension scores of 8,2 and 8,7 years respectively. on the picture story language test (written) the subject's performance on both presentations of the test ranged between that of a 6 year old and an 11,5 year old. as the results of this study revealed age equivalent scores between 6 and 11 5 years and qualitative interpretations revealed that the subject was performing on a concrete level, the writer administered the wisc-r to ascertain the relationship between the subject's verbal and non-verbal performances. results of the wisc-r revealed that the subject is functioning at an 8,9 year old level verbally and at an 11,83 year old level non-verbally. the overall results, therefore, indicate that all language modalities of the subject are impaired to about the same degree, and that the data is compatible with the assertion that there is a strong common factor underlying the impairments in each specific modality. performances at all language modalities were strongly tied to concrete situations. conclusion the quantitative and qualitative analyses of the battery of tests administered to the subject disproved the hypothesis that the communicative skills of the subject would be improved when his i.c.p. was at a higher level. however, the results illustrated that the subject was performing in the concrete-operational stage of piaget's theory thus supporting the possibility that there is a definite factor contributing towards the consistency in the subject's performance throughout testing; namely, a cognitive deficit concomitant with his linguistic impairment. acknowledgements the author wishes to thank ms. b. jacks, clinical tutor and ms c penn lecturer, in the department of speech pathology and audiology of the university of the witwatersrand for their invaluable guidance and support, as well as mr. k. l. allen, the neurosurgeon, and the subject who participated in this study. references 1. allen, k. l. (1980): personal communication. 2 allen, k. l. (1971): a study of the pressure of the cerebrospinal fluid in man by remote monitoring through the skull symposium on bio telemetry. pretoria. 3 allen, k. l. and goldman, ν. i. (1967): phasic pressure characteristics of the cerebrospinal fluid system south africa journal of surgery, 5(4), 151-158. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 90 michelle shapiro 4 crystal, d., fletcher, p. and garman, m. (1976): the grammatical analysis of language disability: a procedure for assessment and remediation. edward arnold. 5. davelaar, e. (1977): formal operational reasoning and its relationship to complex speech patterns and tentative statement use. language and speech, 20, 73-79. 6. de renzi, e. and faglioni, p. (1978): normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the token test. cortex, 14, 41-49. 7. de renzi, e. and ferrari, c. (1978): the reporter's test to detect expressive disturbances in aphasics, cortex, 14, 279-293. 8. de renzi, e. and vignolo, l. a . (1962): the token test to detect receptive disturbances in aphasics. brain, 85, 665-691. 9. eisenson, j. (1971): aphasia in adults: basic considerations. in handbook of speech pathology and audiology. travis, l. (ed.), appleton-century-crofts. 10. eisenson, j. (1973): adult aphasia: assessment and treatment. appleton-century-crofts. 11. flavell, j. h. (1963): the developmental psychology of jean piaget. litton educational publishing. 12. ginsburg, h. and opper, s. (1969): piaget's theory of intellectual development: an introduction. prentice-hall. 13. goldstein, k. (1948): language and language disturbances. grune and stratton. 14. goodglass, h. and kaplan, e. (1972): boston test for aphasia in the assessment of aphasia and related disorders. lea and febiger. 15. inhelder, b. and piaget, j. (1964): the early growth of logic in the child: classification and seriation. routledge and kegan paul. 16. luria, a. r. (1967): higher cortical functions in man. basic books. 17. luria, a . r. and hutton, j. t. (1977): a modern assessment of the basic forms of aphasia. brain and language, 4, 129-151. 18. marshall, r. c., and tompkins, c. a. and phillips, d. s. (1980): effects of scheduling on the communicative assessment of aphasic patients. / . comm. dis., 13(2), 105-114. 19. myklebust, h. r. (1965): development and disorders. of written language: picture story language test. vol. 1./ grune and stratton. / 20. neale, m. d. (1966): neale analysis of reading ability. st. martin's press. 21. penn, c. (1974): a linguistic approach to the detection of minimal language dysfunction in aphasia. / . 5. a. speech & hear assoc. 21, 3-20. ; 22 schuell, h . , jenkins, j. j. and jimenez-pabon, e. (1964): aphasia in adults. harper and row. 1 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) i n t r a c r a n i a l pressure and aphasia 91 23. smith, f. (1975): relation between spoken and written language! in foundations of language development: a multidisciplinary approach. vol. 2. lennenberg, ε. h. and lennenberg, h. (eds.) academic press. 24 wechsler, d. (1974): wechsler intelligence scale for childrenrevised. the psychological corporation. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 43 a preliminary investigation of the pragmatic abilities of a group of language disordered children lindy rosen, bsc (logopaedics) (cape town) rosemary keen, μ sc (london) department of logopaedics university of cape town, cape town abstract the communicative acts, conversational acts, and breakdown repair abilities of six language-disordered children with a syntactic age of ±3 years old as determined by the larsp (crystal, fletcher, garman: 1976) were investigated. sampling of each subject involved two separate naturalistic interactions with a familiar adult and a language disordered peer. the data obtained from the transcriptions were analysed in terms of linguistic and nonlinguistic behaviours on the communication profile by wollner and geller (1982). specific patterns of deficit and strengths were observed and these trends were related to recent literature in this area. the study emphasises that there is as much heterogeneity in the pragmatic skills as in the other communication skills of language disordered children, although this is due to some extent to the limitations of assessment in naturalistic settings. it also indicates individual areas of pragmatic deficit that require remediation. opsomming die kommunikasiehandeling, gesprekshandeling en herstelmoontlikhede van uitvalle by ses kinders met taalsteurnis, met 'n sintaktiese ouderdom van ± 3 jaar volgens die larsp (crystal, fletcher, garman: 1976), is ondersoek. taalmonsters van elke toetspersoon is verkry deur twee aparte naturalistiese interaksies met 'n bekende volwasse en 'n taalversteurde klasmaat. die data wat verkry is van die transkripsies is geanaliseer in terme van linguistiese en nie-linguistiese gedrag deur die gebruik van "the communication profile" van wollner en geller (1982). bepaalde patrone van uitvalle en sterkpunte is opgemerk en hierdie neigings is vergelyk met onlangse literatuur oor hierdie aspek. die studie beklemtoon dat daar net soveel heterogeniteit in die pragmatiese vermoens van taalversteurde kinders voorkom, as in ander kommunikasie-vermoens, alhoewel dit in '« sekere mate te wyte is aan beperkings van ondersoekmoontlikhede in 'n naturalistiese opset. die studie dui ook op individuele aspekte van pragmatiese uitvalle wat remediering vereis. introduction bates (1976) introduced pragmatics into the field of speech therapy and defined it as 'the rules governing the use of language in context.' subsequent authors, e.g. savich (1983), prinz (1982), prutting &{ kirchner (1983), confirm this description. the study of pragmatics then considers com^rnunicative intentions, presuppositions, social context variables and social organisation of discourse. communication is thus much more than issuing and receiving a message. it deals with issuing a message in the most appropriate form for conveying intended meanings to a particular person for particular effects. muma (1975) verifies that both speaker and listener are activeparticipants in the conversational game. donahue (1983) discusses this familiar metaphor used to illustrate a model of conversational turntaking as a network for reciprocal obligations. she compares the conversational game to that of tennis and notes how conversation is governed by grice's (1975) co-operative principle, whereby both partners agree to work at keeping the conversational ball in play. she explains that just as the most skilled tennis player occasionally misses the ball or does not manage to get it over the net, so too, conversational partners can have communication breakdowns. both partners are then obliged to work at reintegrating the dialogue. co-operative listeners are expected to signal to the speakers when a remark is not understood and speakers are then obliged to revise or repeat the initial utterance. when one player is more skilful than another, the superior player is expected to compensate by assuming greater responsibility for keeping the game going (donahue, 1983). this can be seen in children as young as three or four years old who modify their speech when speaking to babies as opposed, to adults (shatz & gelman (1973) and sachs & devin as cited by fey, leonard & wilcox (1981)). hymes, as cited by roth & spekman (1984a), states that in addition to learning the phonologic, semantic and syntactic rules of language, the child must master the rules that underlie how language is used for the purpose of communication. a developmental sequence of pragmatic acquisition has been described by several authors. wiig & semel (1986) cite various perspectives from which this development can be viewed, while ochs & schieffelin (1979) document the following trends: the child's move away from reliance on the immediate context towards greater reliance on nonsituated knowledge; the child's expanding knowledge of conversation for carrying out particular social acts. creaghead (1982) comments that the current interest in pragmatic development has led to the identification of communication deficits in children which had previously been overlooked or considered outside the realm of speech therapy. numerous studies have recently been carried out on pragmatics in language-disordered children. research has revealed significant conflicting data. gallagher & darnton (1978) and brinton and fujiki (1982) report that the language disordered child uses his language in a qualitatively different way from the normal. in contrast van kleeck & frankel (1981) found that the language disordered children they studied used the linguistic devices they investigated in a manner essentially the same as normal children with a similar developmental trend emerging in their.subjects. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikaeafivykings, vol 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 lindy rosen and r o s e m a r y keen prutting & kirchner (1983) state that simply because the child does not possess linguistic and structural sophistication does not necessarily imply that he does not acknowledge the partnership and meet the obligation of conversation. even if the child does not give the exact linguistic response, he may, for example, be aware that an answer is required. they conclude that, given the restricted range of linguistic options available to the disordered communicator, he may be forced to rely on pragmatic strengths (e.g. repetition of the previous utterance) to engage in co-operative conversation to sustain interaction. prutting & kirchner (1983) describe a group of children who display pragmatic assets which allow them to attain a higher level of communicative competence in the context of otherwise limited linguistic ability. donahue (1983) also cites various studies portraying the young language disordered and older learning-disabled child as eager to fulfil his conversational responsibility while compensating for linguistic inadequacies. it was donahue (1983) who likened'these children to tennis players who have a limited set of strokes and seem to hover on the edge of the interaction, watching for an easy opening into the game. she stresses that by seeking out opportunities for participating that provide nonlinguistic and contextual support, the child gives the appearance of keeping the conversational ball in play but simultaneously he is quite adept at avoiding situations that demand linguistic complexity and conversational initiative. the young language impaired child is thus described by savich (1983) and donahue (1983) as adopting an unassertive style which contributes to a cycle of social and linguistic delay in the child as he develops. as prinz (1982) points out, preliminary evidence indicates that the developmental course in pragmatics precedes greater variation and differentiation in expressive and receptive language skills. it also seems apparent that the non-verbal system of communication assumes a very important integrating role in the social-cognitive development process (prinz 1982). clearly the individual pragmatic behaviours of language disordered children should become part of the language assessment protocol. roth & spekman (1984a) emphasise that whereas the development of formalised pragmatic assessment tools must await a clearer delineation of a normal developmental sequence, it is possible to draw on available empirical and theoretical literature to construct an organisational framework for analysing performance in this area. they stress, however, that assessment in the area of pragmatics is still very much in the experimental stages and that our knowledge of normal developmental sequences\is far from complete. part of the framework for analysing pragmatic skills is the communicative setting. gallagher (1983) cites studies by stark and tallal of language development in normal and specifically language impaired children that have demonstrated that there are several major contextual parameters that seem important for language assessment. familiarity seems to be one of the most important factors, with more social and complex play occurring if young children are familiar with each other (doyle, connolly & rivest, cited by gallagher, 1983). craig (1983) reports that skilled clinicians of language impaired children can increase the frequency with which the child has opportunities for experiencing conversational demands and rules for obtaining representative samples of language use for analysis. : another part of the framework for analysing pragmatic skills is the assessment tool. the literature describes various profiles that have recently emerged for the purpose of analysing and explaining pragmatic abilities of children and adults with varying communicative disorders. the majority of the sources are designed for use with children (dore, gearhart & newman (1978), corsaro (1979), and wollner & geller (1982)) in contrast to penn's profile of communicative appropriateness (1985) that was specifically designed for adults with a degree of linguistic and structural sophistication required of participants. while it is important to study normal non-verbal communication in children for further development of assessment material, prinz (1982) advises that it is also necessary to study language disordered children's use of their limited language skills to determine the characteristics of the cycle of delay to provide guidelines for the determination of global therapy goals on a pragmatic basis. with this in mind, the experimenters chose to obtain samples of conversations from language disordered children for analysis using the available assessment profiles and data on normal and disordered pragmatic development to determine the ways in which pragmatic skills are used by these children, thereby providing information that could possibly be of use in their remediation. method the aim of the study was to determine the range and frequency of conversational acts, communicative acts and response to communication breakdown used by a group of language impaired children in two different communicative settings. these three aspects of pragmatic behaviour were the areas covered by wollner & geller's communication profile (1982) which was identified as being the most appropriate of the available assessment material for the subjects of this study. subjects a group of six english-speaking children was selected from fourteen children at the language unit attached to the department of logopaedics at the university of cape town. all these children had been accepted into the language unit according to strict admission criteria. of relevance here is that their intelligence was within the normal range (assessed by a clinical psychologist); hearing was within normal limits (assessed by an audiologist); there was no severe emotional or behavioural disturbance (assessed by a child psychiatrist), and their language was significantly delayed or deviant in production (at least a year's delay on the larsp) (crystal, fletcher & gar j man 1976). their ages ranged from 4.1 years to 4.11 yearsj with a mean of 4.6 years. \ the ss all had an expressive syntax rated on the larsp as' stage iii merging into stage iv, i.e. a language age of approximately 3 years. there were equal numbers of males and females to reduce any language variations that may occur with sex difference. none of the ss had had therapeutic intervention for pragmatics. data collection the ss were videotape-recorded (national camcorder video camera) and audio-tape-recorded (phillips tape recorder) in two separate naturalistic settings of approximately 10 minutes each in the language unit (lu): ' i — semi-structured interaction between the lu's speech therapist and each s the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a preliminary investigation of t h e p r a g m a t i c abilities of a group of language disordered children 45 ii — unstructured interaction between each s and a lu peer with a similar expressive language age. the usual lu play and toy material was made available in both settings. roth & spekman (1984b) note that meaningful, familiar contexts are essential for spontaneous language sampling, as are the provision of motivating activities. these authors state that the ideal assessment process should involve a variety of contexts and that this can be fulfilled by varying communicative partners. the adult partner in interaction i was able to control to some extent in the semi-structured interaction the potential for a variety of communicative and conversational acts and examples of communication breakdown, whereas the interaction between each pair of language impaired children was entirely spontaneous. analysis of the data each interaction was transcribed according to ochs's (1979) criteria for transcription. the identified behaviours were then analysed in detail as linguistic and non-linguistic behaviours in both communicative settings on the following profile: 1. communication profile (wollner & geller, 1982) (appendix 1) this profile evolved from wollner and geller's interest in describing the salient features of the communication performance of a group of language impaired children (wollner & geller, 1982). 1.1 communicative acts this section of the profile is an adaptation of the work done by dore, gearhart & newman (1978) in their profile of illocutionary forces which emerged from the study of normal 3 year-olds in a nursery school setting and involved the same naturalistic sampling as the present study. the'communicative acts that the child achieves by using language are grouped into those that convey content, that regulate conversation and that express attitudes. each category is divided into subtypes, for example, communicative acts conveying content can be requests, comments, responses or performatives. definitions for each type are provided in appendix 2. the non-linguistic communicative acts identified on the profile are the gestures, intonation patterns, facial expressions and/or vocalizations that either substitute for linguistic behaviour or are required by the listener for comprehension of the verbal message. 1.2 conversational acts this section is an adaptation of corsaro's profile on turn taking abilities (corsaro(1979)) which was designed for use in adult/child and peer/peer exchanges in children from 2 vi to 5 years. wollner & geller (1982) identified three main functions within this section: topic initiation, topic extension, and topic termination. definitions of these can be found in appendix 3. 1.3 communicative breakdown wollner & geller (1982) state that the ability to repair and to signal communication breakdown is often more critical for the language impaired child than it is for normals, as language die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafivykins, vol. 36, 1989 impaired children are more often participants in communicative failure or disruption. the two types of behaviour in this section are those concerned with repair offered by the speaker and requested by the listener (appendix 1). wollner & geller (1982) note the following advantages of use of this profile: provision is made for conveyance of communicative acts via nonlinguistic devices as well as linguistically. information is included in the profile that can be applied to prelinguistic communicative development as well as to more sophisticated language development. this is very suitable for language disordered children with a young language age. statistical treatment of data the frequency of behaviours on the communication profile completed for each subject were computed and subjected to x2 analysis of significance of difference to determine whether any of the subjects was significantly different from his/her peers in any of the pragmatic jsehaviours, and whether any of the behaviours was used significantly more or less than any of the others by any of the subjects. results and discussion the results of this investigation reveal that the six subjects used a wide range of conversational acts, communication acts, and responses to communication breakdown, although some behaviour categories were not exhibited by every subject. there were, however, many significant differences between the frequencies of use of the various pragmatic behaviours, and also many significant differences between the subjects. several trends could be identified and these generally conform with those described in the literature. table 1: range of communicative acts for all subjects in interactions i and ii subjects a β c d ε f total requests i 1 3 11** 1 3 5 24 ii 2 5 11* 4 3 14** 39 comments i 6 >15 25 15 6 24 44** 110 ii 13 >15 25 11 22 25 31 127* responses i 45 58 57 93** 54 52 359** ii 18 10 8 12 17 7 72 performatives i 0 0 0 3 5* 2 10 ii 1 10** 0 3 1 3 18 organizational devices i 2 2 3 6 3 13* 29 ii 14 24 3* 17 14 24 96* expressives i 3 7 7 8 10 1 36 ii 2 4 1 1 1 3 12 others i 46 19 25 26 29 8 153 ii 89** 16 7 16 23 13 164** sub-totals i 103 104 118 143 128 125 721 ii 139 94 41** 75 84 95 528 total 242 198 159 218 212 220 1249 ** p<0.01 *p<0,05 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 lindy rosen and r o s e m a r y keen table 1 exhibits the wide range of behaviours used by each subject in both interactions. it is clear, however, that in interaction i, responses were used significantly more than any other category by all the subjects as a group (p <0.01), and that performatives were barely used. in fact three subjects never used any at all. in interaction ii it is comments, and o t h e r s ' that were used significantly more than all the other categories (p <0.01) except organizational devices by all the ss as a group, and expressives as well as performatives that were hardly used at all by most of the ss. these results are not unexpected when considering the level of language development of the ss and the nature of the interactions. rees (1978) and creaghead (1982) document a pragmatic trend in language-impaired children of having a limited range of communication intents in comparison to normally developing children and the tendency for limited use of a variety of speech acts in that kind of partnership. table 2: variation w i t h i n linguistic responses for all subjects in interaction i and ii subjects a β c d ε f total acknowledgements i 17 0 3 2 0 0 22 ii 0 0 0 4 1 1 6 response to question i 0 41* 40* 85** 42* 43* 251** ii 0 0 1 6 3 0 10 clarification i 9 16 9 3 0 8 45 ii 16 10 3 2 12 6 49 totals i 26 57 52 90 42 51 318 ii 16 10 4 12 16 7 65 ** p<0.01 *p<0,05 if one looks at the subcategories of by far the largest category of communicative acts (responses in interaction i) and separates these into linguistic and non-linguistic responses, it becomes clearer why this form of interaction was so frequently used. table 2 shows that for every child except subject a, linguistic responses to questions were significantly the most predominant form of response, although the level of significance varied (p<0.01 and p<0.05) (subject a, in fact, used linguistic responses to questions least of all of his response types.) remembering that their communicative intents are usually limited, these language impaired subjects used this means of keeping the conversational b^ll in play, taking turns where required by modifying the adult's interrogative into a declarative sentence. their non-verbal responses were occasionally indicative of a failure to comprehend the question form. table 3: linguistic/non-linguistic responses for all subjects in interactions i and π subjects a β c d ε f total linguistic i 26 57* 52* 90** 42* 51* 318 "ii 16 10 4 12 16 7 65 non-linguistic i 19** 1 5 3 12 1 41 ii 2 , 0 4 0 1 0 7 totals i 45 58 57 93 54 52 359** ii 18 10 8 12 17 7 72 ** p < 0 01 * p < 0 05 there were other means by which the subjects in this study fulfilled their communicative obligations with limited linguistic skills. when the communicative function of non-linguistic behaviours such as symbolic noise or gesture was clear, these were included in the relevant subcategory in part i of the profile. it turned out that all of these except two (both descriptions), were responses to questions. table 3 indicates the ratio of linguistic to non-linguistic communicative acts used by each subject as responses in both interactions. significantly more linguistic behaviours were used by all the children except subject a in interaction i (p<0.05 or 0.01) where subject a used significantly more non-linguistic responses (p<0.01). these findings correlate with a specific pragmatic disability as described by prutting & kirchner (1983) and by shavakis & greenfield (1982) who state that even when the child acknowledges the partnership of a communicative action and is relatively sensitive to his obligation as a participant, what continues to interfere with his communication are higher level linguistic operations. table 4: variation w i t h i n the "other" category for all subjects in interactions i and π. subjects a β c d ε f total unintelligible i 36 17 20 20 24 16 123** ii 53 16 3 9 21 10 112** incomplete utterance i 1 2 0 0 0 1 4 ii 4 0 0 0 0 0 4 laughter i 4 0 5 0 2 1 12 ii 12 0 4 / 1 1 1 19 screaming i 2 0 0 0 2 0 4 ii 12 0 , 0 1 0 2 15 symbolic noise i 3 0 0 6 1 0 10 ii 8 0 0 5 1 0 14 totals i 46 19 25 26 29 8 153, ii 89** 16 7 16 23 13 164 (no response) 22 j i 3 0 0 7 11 1 22 j ii 0 0 0 2 0 0 2 ι ** p<0.01 * ρ <0.05 there were more non-linguistic strategies used by the children that become evident when the content of the category of o t h e r s ' is looked at in table 4 which is seen to contain several non-linguistic communicative acts in addition to the nonlinguistic entries in the other categories. the o t h e r ' category on wollner & geller's (1982) profile was found to be inadequate for this study in providing space for unintelligible and uninterpretable utterances only. it was therefore modified by combining unintelligible and uninterpretable and adding linguistic and non-linguistic subcategories that were found to be used by the subjects in ways that could not easily be itemized elsewhere on the profile. the additions are shown in table 4. the fact that in interaction ii the category of o t h e r ' was used considerably more than any other category and highly significantly more (p<0.01) than four of the other categories (see table 1) is now explained. within this category are a large number of unintelligible utterances that could not be categothe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 47 a preliminary investigation of t h e p r a g m a t i c abilities of a group of l a n g u a g e disordered children rized as any other communicative act nor did they specifically introduce or extend the topic of conversation. they served instead to perpetuate the interaction. subject a produced the largest number of unintelligible utterances, and it is interesting that he used more of these in interaction ii, while all the others used more in interaction i where they were responding to an adult. it was also subject a, who was the least advanced linguistically on the larsp profile (crystal, fletcher & garman (1976), who used symbolic noise, laughter and screaming more than the other children, and this behaviour could be included in those back-channel behaviours that fey & leonard (1983) describe as serving to maintain social contact, and at the same time avoiding taking the conversational floor. if one adds exclamations to these behaviours (the only entry within the expressive category for all the subjects) it is apparent that all the children used this black-channelling as a means for fulfilling their social responsibility. referring again to table 1, subject c used highly significantly more requests than his peers in interaction i (<0.01) and significantly more than any of the others (p <0.05) except subject f who used the most in interaction ii. subject c, however, also used significantly fewer organizational devices (p<0.05) than the rest in interaction ii and in fact his total number of 159 communicative acts is considerably smaller than all the other children whose totals ranged from 198 to 242 acts. table 5: variation w i t h i n organizational devices for all subjects in interactions i and ii subjects a β c d ε f total attention-getters i 0 0 1 1 1 11 14 ii 10 22 2 3 10 19 66* speaker selection i 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 ii 0 0 0 2 0 0 2 boundary markers , i , 2 2 2 5. 2 2 15 ii 4 2 1 12 3 5 27 / politeness markers 1 1 i 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 ii 0 0 0 ' 0 1 0 1 totals [ i 2 2 3 6 3 13 29 ii 14 24 3 17 14 24 96 * p> 0.05 j the children in this study used one organizational device much more than any other. the bulk of the entries for interaction ii in the organizational device category in table 5 are attention-getters (used significantly more than other organizational devices by all the children as a group in interaction ii), indicating the importance of this behaviour in the subjects' repertoire of communicative skills in certain communicative contexts. in interaction 1 the attention of the adult was focused exclusively on the child, and all the subjects, were aware of this, while in interaction ii both children's attention was focused primarily on themselves in play, with one or other child being content occasionally to play alone without involvement with the other member of the dyad. attentiongetters were therefore necessary to re-initiate the communicative interaction. mctear (1985) & ochs, schieffelin & piatt (1979) discuss how lexical attention-getters are used by children as young as two years old. mueller (1972) showed that the frequency of occurrence of attention-getter increases with age, and found it to be a function of the child's developing communicative competence. in contrast, only one of the six children, subject e, employed a politeness marker, and this child used it only twice in spite of numerous potential occasions for him and the other subjects. leonard, nippold, & anastopoulous (1982) elaborate on the emergence of politeness markers and relate them to the ability of the child to adopt the perspective of the other person, but there is no available information on this skill in language impaired children. if one compares the total number of utterances produced by each subject in each interaction in table 1, it is only subject a who apparently talked a lot more in interaction ii. it seems he is therefore the only child who conformed with fey, leonard & wilcox's (1981) language impaired subjects who spoke more frequently with their peers than with others, but this observation must be viewed in the light of the fact that he produced many more non-linguistic utterances than the other subjects (see table 4). table 6: topic initiation & extension for all subjects i n interactions i and ii subjects a β c d ε f total initiation i 0 0 2 1 3 12 18 ii 12 13 7 9 16 15 71 total 12 13 9 10 19 27 90 extension i 37 94 88 96 88 90 493 ii 27 64 26 57 46 91 311 total 64 158 114 153 134 181 804 although all six subjects used more responses as communicative acts than comments with the adult, and reversed this trend in the interaction with their peer (see table 1), it is apparent in table 6 that when the subjects' interactions are looked at as conversational acts, all the children were extenders of topics rather than initiators in both interactions. this appears not to conform with the above findings about comments being dominant in interaction ii, but if one considers that topic extension includes more than responses, it is clear there is no anomaly. the subjects were in fact confirming the findings of several authors including donahue (1983) who reports that the stronger communicative partner generally assumes responsibility for maintaining the dialogue, and siegel, cunningham and van der spuy cited by fey & leonard (1983), who describe how, in interaction with familiar adults, language impaired children are less likely to iniate interaction. table 7 shows the variety of ways in which the subjects extended the topics in both interactions. it is clear that the majority of extensions at a highly significant level (p<0.01) occurred for all the children as a group in both interactions as topic relevant extensions when they were related to the content of the previous speaker's act, but went further by adding to it. further, these topic relevant extensions were used considerably more in interaction i by each subject. subject d was the only subject who used more acknowledgements than topic relevant extensions in interaction i, although subject b's use of each was almost equal. wollner & geller (1982) stress the important notion that the child's ability to sustain the topic die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykin/js, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 lindy rosen a n d r o s e m a r y keen with relevant contributions over extended turns depends on the conversational partner and the partner's sensitivity to the child's cognitive and linguistic level. table 7: variation w i t h i n topic extension for all subjects in interactions i and π. subjects a β c d ε f total acknowledgements i 5* 33 17 56 28 27 166 ii 6 2 0 17 5 9 39 topic relevant i 20 35 45 32 45 63 240** ii 7 19 11 30 21 52 140** topic shift i 0 1 2 0 1 0 4 ii 1 24** 4 2 0 4 35 off topic i 1 3 4 2 4 0 14 ii 3 8 0 0 0 3 14 resume topic i 1 1 0 0 0 0 2 ii 1 5 0 2 7 15 30 clarification request i 0 1 6 1 0 0 8 ii 0 2 6 2 2 2 14 clarification response i 9 17 11 3 7 0 47 ii 7 3 4 2 10 6 32 totals i 36 91 85 94 85 90 481 ii 25 63 25 55 45 91 304 ** p < 0 01 *p<0,05 all the children except subject c used more comments than verbal responses in interaction ii (see table 1) and all the subjects initiated more topics in interaction ii than in interaction i (see table 6). these findings support those on fey, leonard & wilcox (1981) who found that the language impaired subjects they studied became more assertive when interacting with other language impaired children. one child, subject f, used almost the same number of comments and responses with the adult, and many more comments than the other children in both interactions (see table 1). he was also the only child who initiated as many topics as those he extended (see table 6). it is important to note that this child was the oldest and most advanced linguistically on his larsp profile (crystal, fletcher & garman (1976)). \ when the subjects were asked for clarification of their utterances, it was more frequently by the adult in interaction i, but in interaction ii, all the children except subject a signalled a need for repair (see table 7). table 8 shows that subject a was also the only child who responded to a request for clarification with a nonverbal response in either interaction, and his only other attempt to repair was by repetition of his unintelligible utterances without attempting to modify them but all of them utteranced without attempting to modify them but all of them except subject d also attempted some other means of clarification, with subject β producing a wide repertoire. the children were generally very tolerant of clarification requests, and occasionally responded with up to four successive clarification attempts. they also requested acknowledgement after their clarification responses to ensure that their message had been understood. gallagher (1977) also found that regardless of language age, no subject ignored requests for clarification, although the responses were dependent on the level of his structural knowledge. donahue, pearl & bryan (1980) on the other hand, report the trend that language impaired children are less likely to initiate repair of communication breakdown and hence appear to be less co-operative communication partners. this is confirmed in the present study if the latter authors' finding refers to requests for clarification (see table 7). table 8: responses to clarification requests for all subjects in interactions i and π. subjects a β c d ε f total no repair i 2 4 0 1 0 0 7 ii 4 1 0 0 0 1 6 repetition i 2 4 5 2 3 0 16 ii 3 1 3 2 4 4 17 syntac/semantic revision i 0 1 3 0 2 0 6 ii 0 0 0 0 5 0 5 paraphrase i 0 0 1 0 0 0 1 ii 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 part repetition i 0 2 0 0 1 0 2 ii 0 • 1 0 0 0 0 1 unintelligible i 0 1 0 0 1 0 2 ii 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 multiple repetition i 0 2 0 0 0 0 2 ii 0 0 0 0 1 0 1 incomplete clarification i 0 1 0 0 0 0 . 1 ii 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 request acknowledgement i 0 2 2 0 0 0 4 ii 0 0 1 0 0 1 2 non-verbal clarification i 5 0 0 0 0 0 5 ii 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 ! totals i i 9 17 11 3 7 0 47 ii 7 3 4 2 10 6 32 it is worth noting that all six subjects differed significant! ly frifm all of their peers in at least orte type of communicative act. ifhis serves to remind us of the heterogeneity of the com-, municative behaviour of language impaired children, but may also be due to the sampling procedure of this study. roth &1 spekman (1984a) discuss various limitations of assessment in naturalistic settings. they comment on how analysis of the data is always limited to what a child produces, that the mere absence of a particular communicative intent or failure to initiate new topics cannot necessarily be seen as an indication that such a skill is not part of the child's repertoire, and that the presence of a particular communicative behaviour may not be demonstrated with sufficient frequency to assess its adequacy. these limitations apply to assessment within a variety of naturalistic settings, and the results from the present investigation should be considered in this framework. the relevance of these comments can be highlighted, for example, by the results obtained regarding politeness markers which may well have been used more by these children in settings with less familiar communicative partners. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a preliminary investigation of t h e p r a g m a t i c abilities of a group of l a n g u a g e disordered children 49 a f u r t h e r c o m m e n t h a s b e e n m a d e b y f e y , l e o n a r d & wilcox ( 1 9 8 1 ) w h o s t a t e t h a t i n c o n v e r s a t i o n , e a c h p a r t i c i p a n t b r i n g s a c o m b i n a t i o n of p e r c e p t u a l , social, cognitive a n d linguistic skills to t h e task. t h e y w a r n a g a i n s t v i e w i n g a n y of t h e s e v a r i a b l e s i n i s o l a t i o n . g a l l l a g h e r ( 1 9 8 3 ) c o n f i r m s t h i s o p i n i o n a n d i n t r o d u c e s i s s u e s of p e r s o n a l i t y t h a t n e e d to b e c o n s i d e r e d i n d i s c u s s i n g a c h i l d ' s c o m m u n i c a t i v e c o m p e t e n c e . she a l s o cites e x t e n s i v e e v i d e n c e t h a t suggests t h a t t h e r e is a significant i n t e r r e l a t i o n s h i p b e t w e e n l a n g u a g e u s e v a r i a b i l i t y a n d e v e r y a s p e c t of p h y s i c a l c o n t e x t t h a t p r o b a b l y c a n , b u t w i l l n o t n e c e s s a r i l y , effect a p a r t i c u l a r c h i l d ' s l a n g u a g e u s e ( g a l l a g h e r 1 9 8 3 ) . s h e c o n c e d e s , h o w e v e r , t h a t s a m p l i n g a c r o s s a l i m i t e d n u m b e r of c o n t e x t u a l e n v i r o n m e n t s m e e t s t h e p r a c t i cal r e q u i r e m e n t of efficiency ( g a l l a g h e r 1 9 8 3 ) . c o n c l u s i o n r e s u l t s of t h e p r e s e n t i n v e s t i g a t i o n serve t o e m p h a s i s e t h e h e t e r o g e n e i t y t h a t e x i s t s i n c h i l d r e n w i t h l a n g u a g e d i s o r d e r s a n d i n p r a g m a t i c t r e n d s a s t h e y r e l a t e t o s y n t a c t i c developm e n t . t h e s t u d y a l s o h i g h l i g h t s t h e v a r i e t y of p r a g m a t i c abilities d i s p l a y e d b y t h e l a n g u a g e d i s o r d e r e d c h i l d r e n s t u d i e d a n d exemplifies b o t h p a t t e r n s of s t r e n g t h a n d w e a k n e s s e s a s exhib i t e d b y t h e m a n d a s r e p o r t e d i n t h e l i t e r a t u r e . it h a s b e e n e s t a b l i s h e d h e r e t h a t t h e s e c h i l d r e n u s e a v a r i e t y of comm u n i c a t i v e a c t s a n d n o n l i n g u i s t i c c o m m u n i c a t i o n t o comp e n s a t e for l i m i t e d l i n g u i s t i c skills a n d t h a t t h e y u n d e r s t a n d t h a t c o m m u n i c a t i o n is a s y m b o l s h a r i n g s y s t e m t h a t r e q u i r e s c o o p e r a t i v e p a r t n e r s . i d e n t i f i c a t i o n of e a c h c h i l d ' s p a t t e r n s of i n t e r a c t i o n w i t h i n t h e b o u n d a r i e s of t h e s a m p l i n g p r o c e d u r e s u s e d i n t h e s t u d y a d d s t o t h e a s s e s s m e n t d a t a for t h e i n c o r p o r a t i o n i n t o t h e t h e r a p y g o a l s for i n c r e a s i n g t h e o v e r a l l effectiven e s s of t h e c h i l d ' s c o m m u n i c a t i o n . a c k n o w l e d g e m e n t s s i n c e r e t h a n k s a r e e x t e n d e d to t h e s p e e c h a n d h e a r i n g ther a p i s t a t t h e l a n g u a g e u n i t a t g r o o t e s c h u u r h o s p i t a l for h e r c o o p e r a t i v e p a r t i c i p a t i o n i n t h e s t u d y , t o t h e c h i l d r e n a t t h e l a n g u a g e u n i t w h o w e r e t h e s u b j e c t s i n t h e i n v e s t i g a t i o n , a n d to t h e t e a c h i n g m e t h o d s u n i t a t t h e u n i v e r s i t y of c a p e t o w n for t h e i r v i d e o t a p e r e c o r d i n g service. / r e f e r e n c e s bates, b. language and context: the acquisition of pragmatics. new york: academic press incorporated, 1976. brinton, b. and fujiki, m. a comparison of request response sequences in the discourse of normal and language disordered children. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 47(1), 57-62, 1982. | corsaro, w. a. the development of social cognition in preschool children: implications for language learning.topics in language disorders 1(2), 77-95 1979. craig, h.k. applications of pragmatic language models for intervention. in t.m. gallagher and c a prutting (eds), pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. california: college hill press, 1983. creaghead, n. children with disorders of pragmatics in jv irwin (ed), pragmatics: the role in language development. california: fox point publishing ltd, 1982. crystal, d„ fletcher, p., and garman, m. the grammatical analysis of language disability: a procedure for assessment and remediation. london: edward arnold, 1976. donahue, m. learning disabled children as conversational partners. topics in language disorders, 15, 25-37, 1983. donahue, j., gearhart, m., & newman, d. conversation competence in learning disabled children: responses to uninform'ative messages. applied psycholinguistics, 1, 387-403, 1980. dore, j., gearhart, m., and newman, d. the structure of nursery school conversation. in κ nelson (ed), children's language, vol 1. new york: gardner press, 371-372, 1978. fey, m., and leonard l. pragmatic skills of children with specific language impairment. in t.m. gallagher and c a prutting (eds), pragmatic assessment and intervention in language. california: college hill press, 1983. fey, m., leonard, l„ and wilcox, k.^speech style modifications of language impaired children. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 46, 91-96, 1981. gallagher, t.m. revision behaviours in the speech of normal children developing language. journal of speech and hearing research, 20, 305-318, 1977. gallagher, t.m. pre-assessment: a procedure for accommodating language use variability. in t.m. gallagher and c aprutting (eds), pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. california: college hill press, 1983. gallagher, t.m. darnton β a. conversational aspects of the speech of language disordered children: revision behaviours. journal of speech and hearing research, 21, 118-135, 1978. grice, h.p. logic and conversation. in p. cole and j. morgan (eds), studies in syntax and semantics, speech acts (3). new york: academic press 1975. leonard, l., nippold, m., and anastopoulous, a. development in the use of and understanding of polite forms in children. journal of speech and hearing research, 25, 193-202, 1982. mctear, m.f. children's conversations. oxford: blackwell publishers, oxford, 1985. mueller, e. the maintenance of verbal exchanges between young children. child development, 43(3), 930-938, 1972. muma, j.r. the communication game: dump and play. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 40, 296-307, 1975. ochs, e., and schieffelin, b. (eds). developmental pragmatics. new york: academic press 1979. ochs, e. transcription as theory. in ε ochs and b. schieffelin (eds), developmental pragmatics. new york: academic press, 43-72 1979. ochs, e„ schieffelin, b., and piatt, m. propositions across utterances and speakers. in e. ochs and b. schieffelin (eds), developmental pragmatics. new york: academic press, 251-268, 1979. penn, c. the profile of communicative appropriateness. a clinical tool for the assessment of pragmatics. south african journal of communication disorders, 32, 18-25, 1985. prinz, p. development of pragmatics: multiword level. in j v irwin (ed), pragmatics the role in language development. california: fox point publishing ltd, 1982. prutting, c., and kirchner, d. applied pragmatics in τ μ gallagher and c a prutting (eds) pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. california: college hill press, 1983. rees, n. pragmatics of language: applications to normal and disordered language development. in r. schiefelbusch (ed), bases of language intervention. baltimore: university park press 1978. roth, f.p., and spekman, n.j. assessing the pragmatic abilities of children: part 1: organizational framework and assessment parameters. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 42, 211.1984(a). roth, f.p., and spekman, n.j. assessing the pragmatic abilities of children: part 2. guidelines, considerations and specific eva. luation procedures./ournai of speech and hearing disorders, 49, 12-17, 1984(b). savich, p. improving communication competence the role of metapragmatic awareness. topics in language disorders, 15 47, 1983. shavakis, e„ and greenfield, p. the role of new-old information in the verbal expression of language-disordered children. journavof speech and hearing research, 25, 462-467, 1982. shatz, m., and gelman, r. the development of communication skills: modifications in the speech of young children as a function of the listener. monographs of the society for research in child development. 38-47, 1973. van kleeck and frankel, t.l. discourse devices used by language disordered: a preliminary investigation. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 46(3), 250-257, 1981. wiig, e.g. and semel, e. language assessment and intervention for the learning disabled. 2nd edition. ohio: charles merrill publishing company, 1986. wollner, s. and geller, e. methods of assessing pragmatic abilities, in j.v. irwin (ed), pragmatics the rale in language development. california: fox point publishing limited, 1982. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 lindy rosen and r o s e m a r y keen appendix 1 communication profile part 1 child's name sima gerber wollner context: elaine geller dates: i. communicative acts* speaker listener 1. convey content ling non-ling** ling non-ling requests 1. action or object 2. information 3. permission 4. clarification b. comments 1. labels 2. descriptions 3. attributions 4. rules 5. explanations c. responses 1. acknowledgements 2. to questions 3. clarifications d. performatives 1. claims 2. jokes 3. protests 2. regulate conversation organizational devices 1. attention getters 2. speakers selections 3. boundary markers 4. politeness markers 3. express attitudes expressives 1. express emotions 2. exclamations 4. other a. uninterpretable b. unintelligible total communicative act! summary · speaker yes no 1. the child expresses a range of communicative act types. 2. the child uses non-linquistic devices to communicate acts. 3. the child conveys communicative acts clearly. 4. the child uses a variety of forms to express communicative act types. 5. the child uses appropriate forms to express communicative act types. 6. the child uses direct/indirect forms to express communicative act types. * (adapted from the work of dore, gearhart & newman, 1978.) ** (non-linguistic includes gaze, gesture, intonation, facial expression and vocalization.) communication profile part ii child's name sima gerber wollner context: elaine geller dates: ii. conversational acts* speaker listener ling non-ling ling non-ling 1. initiate interaction/ topic a. establish joint attention/ activity/reference. 2. extend interaction/ topic a. acknowledgement b. topic relevant c. topic shift d. off topic e. resume topic f. clarification request g. clarification response 3. terminate interaction/ topic summary speaker yes no 1. the child initiates topics/interaction. 2. the child initiates topics in an appropriate way 3. the child extends topics/interaction. 4. the child extends topics with appropriate/relevant conversational contributions. 5. the child uses a variety of conversational acts to extend topic. 6. the child extends topics/interaction over successive turns * (adapted from the work of corsaro, 1979) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a preliminary investigation of the p r a g m a t i c abilities of a group of language disordered children 51 appendix 2 definitions of communicative acts 1. convey content a. requests involve soliciting information or actions. 1. requests for action/object involve the speaker directing the listener to perform an action, e.g., gimme ride. 2. requests for information involve the speaker obtaining solicited information from another participant, e.g., what did you watch on tv? 3. requests for permission involve the speaker seeking permission to perform an action, e.g., can i go out? 4. requests for clarification involve the speaker seeking clarification of another person's remark, e.g., what did you say? b. comments include descriptions and/or identification of observable events in the environment and statements which report facts or sta'te rules. 1. labels involve identification of objects, events, persons, etc., e.g., that's a car. 2. description predicate information about events, properties or locations of people or objects, e.g., it fall down. 3. attributions report a speaker's beliefs about another person's internal state, e.g., it's too hard for him. 4. rules state procedure, definitions, "social rules" or facts, e.g., we don't fight in school. 5. explanations involve the speaker stating reasons, causes, or justification for someone's behaviour, e.g., i hit him because i don't like him. c. responses involve the speaker providing solicited in-' formation to another speaker's prior remark or the acknowledgement of another speaker's prior utterance. 1. acknowledgements involve the speaker recognizing another participant's prior remark using verbal fillers which merely accompany the interaction without providing new/or additional information, e.g., right or yeah. 2. responses to questions involve the speaker providing the solicited information to a wh-question or yes/no question. 3. clarifications involve clarification of the speaker's previous remark usually in direct response to a clariy fication request. j d. performatives accomplish acts and establish facts by being said. | 1. claims involve the speaker establishing his rights, e.g., that's mine; i go, first. 2. jokes involve the speaker causing some humorous effect by stating incongruous information which is usually false, e.g., we throwed the soup in the ceiling. 3. protests involve thejspeaker expressing some objections to another individual's behaviour, e.g., stop that, no. 2. regulate conversation a. organizational devices accompany an interactional exchange and control the conversation flow and maintain personal contact with the other speaker(s). 1. attention getters involve the speaker attempting to ι gain the listener's attention, e.g. hey, jonn. 2. speaker selection involve labelling the next speaker's turn, e.g., you go, your turn. 3. boundary markers are used to mark openings, closings, or shifts in the conversation, e.g., hi-bye, oh, or bye the way. 4. politeness markers involve the explicit use of polite forms, e.g., thank you, please. 3. express attitudes a. expressives involve the conveying of feelings or attitudes. 1. express emotions involves the speaker conveying emotions, feelings or attitudes, e.g., i hate you. 2. exclamations involve the speaker expressing feelings or attitudes through the use of non-propositional language forms, e.g., wow, yuch! (adapted from the work of dore, gearhart & newman, 1978) appendix 3 definitions of conversational acts 1. initiate interaction/topic a. establish joint attention/activity/reference refers to any act which encourages a focused interaction by at least two participants in terms of a specified activity or a specified content. 1. non-linguistically child shows or offers a toy to the adult; e.g., child points to his milk. 2. linguistically, the child says hey, look at this, or wanna play with play-doll? 2. extend interaction/topic a. acknowledgement is a non-linguistic or linguistic act which is a response to a previous act but doesn't go beyond in terms of content. here, we can include responses to ves/no auestions, as well as responses to nonrequests. for example, child a: i like popeye. child b: right. b. topic relevant acts are related to the content of the previous speaker's act and go beyond or add to the previous act in terms of content. these can be thought of as further contributions to the conversation. for example, mother: look at big bird. child: big bird has feathers. c. topic shift is any act which takes place during a focused interaction where there is a direct attempt to change the topic. in our system, unlike corsaro's. these may or may not be marked formally i.e., l^et's do this now. d. off-topic acts occur within a focused interaction, are not relevant to the topic, and are usually initiated without a formal marker. e. resume topic is any act which refers back to a previously introduced topic. f. clarification requests are acts which call for the clarification, confirmation, or repetition of a preceding act. these usually take the form of interrogatives. for example, what did you say? or what do you mean? g. clarification responses are acts which are direct reactions to clarification reauests. the clarification responses do not go beyond the information or behaviour requested. 3. terminate interaction/topics this refers to any act which leads to the end of a focused interaction. a. non-linguistically, this might involve a physical movement away from the interaction or no response. b. linguistically, it might involve verbal cueing to terminate the interaction, e.g., child: bye-bye child: no more book (adapted from the work of corsaro, 1979) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 36, 1989 \ r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) -2play rirk and • j schoolroom have moved to bigger and better premises at shop 6l the rosebank mews 173 oxford road rosebank jhb. play & schoolroom, specialists in the field of child education have been offering assistance to both professionals and parents for nearly thirty years. their expertise and advice range through pre-school education, perceptual training, primary and remedial education and adult education. play and schoolroom are sole agents for learning development aids which includes an excellent selection of materials of interest to the speech therapist. they also offer an interesting range of aids and books to foster and develop language and communicative skills. their stock of educational books and toys is exceptionally wide. you are invited to view their superb range in their new beautifully laid out showroom. phone 788-1304 po box 52137 (as before) sax0nw0ld fax: 880-1341 2132 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy programme with spastic dysphonia a single case study 31 rutter, m. the development of infantile autism. j. psychol. med. 9, 147-163, 1974. schuler, a.l. aspects of communication. in: fay, w.h. and schuler, a.l., emerging language in autistic children. language intervention series, 5, edward arnold ltd, london, great britain, 1980. tough, j. the development of meaning: a study of children's use of language. george allen and unwin ltd, london, great britain, 1977. wing, l. early childhood autism: clinical, educational and social aspects. a wheaten and co., exeter, great britain, 1976. a stuttering therapy programme with spastic dysphonia a single case study ingrid meyers ba (sp & η th) (witwatersrand) speech therapy department baragwanath hospital denise anderson ba (sp & η th) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract this study was motivated by reported similarities in vocal tract dynamics in stuttering and spastic dysphonia. the effects of a stuttering therapy programme with an adult with spastic dysphonia were observed. subjective and objective measures obtained preand posttherapeutically included a qualitative analysis, laryngographic tracings, and fiberoptic examinations. results showed subtle improvements on all measures suggesting improved laryngeal behaviours. findings are discussed in relation to therapeutic utility. opsomming die motivering vir hierdie ondersoek is gebasseer op die ooreenkomste t.o.v. die dinamiek van die vokale gang tydens hakkel en spastiese disfonie dieuitwerkingvan 'nterapieprogram vir hakkel wat uitgevoer is op 'n pasient met spastiese disfonie, is waargeneem. subjektiewe en objektiewe metings is vooren na-terapeuties van laringografiese afdrukke, fiberoptiese ondersoeke en van 'n kwalitatiewe anahse verkry. resultate dui op 'n subtiele verbetering op alle metings, wat verbeterde laringale werking aandui. die bevindinge word bespreek t. o. v. kliniese bruikbaarheid. of all the human voice disorders, the syndrome of adductor spastic dysphonia has remained the most mysterious, the most poorly understood, and the most resistant to effective treatment, (boone 1972; aronson 1980; reich and till 1983). spastic dysphonia is a rare disorder and literature on the subject is limited, which has led to some confusion with regard to treatment (wolk, 1980). yet most authorities agree on its poor response to therapy procedures. the disorder, first described by traube in 1871 is characterized by "a strained, creaking, choked vocal attack and a tense squeezed voice accompanied by extreme tension of the entire phonatory system." (luchsinger and arnold 1965). various terms have been used to describe the disorder of spastic dysphonia. it has been referred to as 'glottal spasms', 'stammering of the vocal cords' and 'laryngeal stuttering', and has been parallelled to the disorder of stuttering by mccall (1975) and salamy and sessions (1980). stuttering is a more common disorder, and the age of onset and sex distribution are well defined. the two disorders can, however be likened in terms of their variability, development and course, etiological controversy and history of therapeutic failure with high rates of symptom relapse (ingham and andrews 1973; aronson 1980; salamy and sessions 1980). perhaps the abnormality in the physiology of the larynx for stuttering and spastic dysphonia could be considered a major similarity. mccall (1975) found that similarities between spastic dysphonia and the stuttering block included muscle spasms, disturbed muscle tone and involuntary movements e.g. tremor, with normal laryngeal structures evident on laryngologic examination. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 the past decade has brought about a dramatic transition in the theoretical foundation and purpose in the treatment of stuttering. based on the factors which induce fluency in stutterers, remediation has shifted to manipulation of phonation utilizing behaviour modification techniques as a vehicle for establishing fluency (shames and florance 1980). common to all 'fluency-based' programmes are smooth initiation of phonation, decreased rate and continuous phonation and breath flow which facilitates co-ordination of the vocal folds concurrent with the execution of articulatory gestures (miller 1982). schwartz (1976) felt that it was the reduction in stress on the cords which facilitated their laxing and lengthening and enabled their greater bulk to vibrate. the literature on spastic dysphonia does not reflect the same advances regarding treatment approaches. while some authorities have described similar techniques to those constituting 'fluency-based' programmes, no-one has as yet integrated these techniques into a unified therapy programme. in view of the limited investigations into the field of spastic dysphonia, and of the need to explore new avenues for treatment, the writers decided to investigate the effects of a fluency-based programme (developed for stutterers) by shames and florance (1980) on a subject with spastic dysphonia. method subject the subject, s, was an english speaking adult male, aged 34 years whose profession involved much public speaking. his voice problem © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 ingrid meyers and denise anderson began at the age of twenty and since that time he has received treatment in the form of psychotherapy, medication and tradmonal voice therapy, all of which have provided little relief from the disorder. no farruly history of spastic dysphoria has been reported. s s voice was characterized by breaks in phonation caused by an intermittent strained-strangled and highly tense voice with reduced modal pitch and voice intensity. reflex actions of phonation were intact and thus laughing and coughing were carried out normally. the symptoms became more prominent as the communicative demands of speech were increased. procedure pre-1ntervent10n voice measures pre-intervention samples were obtained over two days prior to the initiation of treatment. these samples were analyzed subjectively and objectively. all measures were obtained in a sound proof recording room, in the phonetic laboratory, department of linguistics at the university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. the three voice samples included: (1) sustained phonation of (i:) for 5 sees. (2) reading the first paragraph of the fairbanks rainbow passage (fairbanks 1940). (3) a spontaneous speech sample. a) subjective evaluation the voice was recorded on the uher 4200 stereo tape recorder. two independent raters judged the severity of the voice samples. a rating scale adapted from aronson et al. (1968) and wilson (1979) was utilized for the qualitative analysis (see appendix i). severity was measured by classifying the subject's voice on a severity continuum based on aronson et al (1968). the two raters who rated blind as to whether the recordings were preor post-therapeutic, calculated the frequency of occurrence of strained, squeezed, staccato or effortful phonation (aronson 1980) for both reading and spontaneous speech. this constituted a measure of percentage and strained-strangled syllables (%ss), which was to be used as an equivalent of percentage stuttered syllables used for stutterers. this has been found to be a good indicator for treatment effectiveness (perkins et al. 1974). finally, a speaking and reading rate was calculated by counting the number of syllables spoken in one minute. this was to constitute a measure of syllables per minute (spm), which is necessary for determining the desired rate to be reached in the therapy programme (shames and florance, 1980). b) objective evaluation the following measures were obtained by the principal phonetician in the department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand: i) laryngographic tracings. wechsler (1977) writes that laryngographic tracings provide a qualitative description of the voice, and show preferred qualitative estimates of vocal fold regularity. using a fourcin laryngograph and voicescope, the laryngographic procedure was carried out in the manner described by wechsler (1977) and kelman (1981). hirano (1981) writes that tracings should be obtained from both the steady position of sustained vowels, and the transitional phases of phonation. the output of the laryngograph and voicescope was simultaneously recorded on a mingograf inkjet recorder. each laryngograph was segmented into phonation stretches of 100 m/sec. the stretches were analyzed according to preselected categories from the features presented by wechsler (1977) and kelman (1981), and a frequency count based on visualizing these categories was used (see appendix ii). ii) fiberoptic examination. this measure was employed to observe the laryngeal behaviours directly during connected speech. aronson (1980) and chapey and salzberg (1981) maintain that fiberoptic examination is the only means by which one can observe and describe vocal fold behaviour, as well as co-ordinate the visual and auditory aspects of voice production. a flexible fiberscope connected by a c-mount adaptor to a type 4az olympus omi camera was used for the fiberoptic examination. the tape recorder was prepared to run concurrently with the examination so as to record all utterances. the vocal folds were examined at rest, during deep breathing and during a spontaneous speech sample. they were also examined during the prolongation of (i:) at a comfortable loudness and pitch level. the audio tape recordings corresponding to each slide, derived from the fiberoptic examination, were transcribed and two independent raters judged the perceptual characteristics of each slide. intervennon programme the intervention was carried out daily by one of the writers over a 3-week period. the programme employed was the 'stutter-free' speech programme (shames and florance 1980). based on the principles of operant conditioning to modify behaviour, the programme involves five overlapping stages which are systematically scheduled. the client is taught to deliberately control the rate of speech and to control the segmenting of speech acts so that there is continuous phonation and airflow between and within words. continuous phonation with normal rate and prosody become the goals for treatment. monitored speech is emphasized and the new speaking skills are transferred into the patient's entire talking day by moving up a situation hierarchy. unmonitored speech eventually replaces monitored speech (shames and florance 1980). the following alterations were made to shames and florance's (1980) original 'stutter-free' speech programme: j — the desired parameter of fluency was exchanged with that of efficient voice. | — the passive airflow technique, described by schwartz (1976) was used to assist s. in initiating phonation. the rationale for, and nature of the treatment programme was explained to s., and both reading and speaking were practised at varying rates of speech. a home programme was included as an adjunct to the therapy programme. post-intervention measures over the two days immediately after the therapy programme, both subjective and objective evaluations were carried out following the identical procedures to that of the pre-intervention evaluation. results interrater reliability was high for all measures (93-100%). the overall severity of the voice and the samples corresponding to the fiberoptic slides were discussed until some agreement was reached. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a stuttering therapy programme with spastic dysphonia a single case study 33 subjective evaluation a ) qualitative analysis and overall vocal severity: table 1 indicates that while there was some improvement in the reading passage, the improvement was not marked. however, there was an overall improvement for the speech sample and prolonged vowel. table 1 indicates a summary of the results obtained for all three voice samples preand post-therapeutically. table 1 mean ratings from the preand post-therapy qualitative analysis of 10 vocal parameters rated on a 7 pt. equal interval rating scale with 1 = normal and 7 = deviant, as well as overall severity of the vocal pattern. vocal parameter /i:/ reading 30 sees. spontaneous speech vocal parameter pre post pre post pre post 1) laryngeal tension 6 1 7 5,5 7 3 2) laryngeal tone rough/hoarse 4 1,5 2,5 3 3 3,5 3) voice tremor 6 4 3 3,5 4,5 4 4) intermittent strain-strangle 3 1 5,5 5 5 2,5 5) constant strain-strangle 2 1 2 1 2 1 6) loudness 4 1 1 1,5 2 1,5 7) pitch 5,5 3,5 2,5 3 4,5 2,5 8) pitch breaks 4 1 3 1 3 1 9) rate ' 1,5 2 1,5 5 10) overall vocal efficiency 4,5 2 7 5 7 2 overall severity moderate mild severe moderate moderate mild table 2 preand post-therapeutic frequency count of strained syllables (%ss); total number of syllables and rate of speech in spm for reading and spontaneous speech. reading 30 sec. spontaneous speech pre post pre post %ss. 14,8% 8,6% 15,4% 0% total syllables 128 |128 84 63 rate (spm) 200 j160 190 150 b) frequency of strained vocal behaviours (%ss) and rate of speech (spm): table 2 indicates a reduction in %ss post-therapeutically, as well as a reduction in overall rate of speech for both the reading and speech samples. objective evaluations a) laryngographic analysis: a summary of the results for the prolonged (i:), reading and spontaneous speech samples on each category of the laryngograph is presented in table 3. a frequency count for each type of waveform per phonation stretch is expressed in terms of a percentage. table 3 preand post laryligographic measures for prolonged /hi, reading and spontaneous speech, (n = total number 1 ι waveform category prolonged /i:/ reading spontaneous speech 1 ι waveform category n=23 pre n=23 post η=101 pre n=126 post n=35 pre n=50 post a) variability per phonation stretch 13% 4,3% 70,3% 71,4% 77,1% 64% b) shape of lx: i) double/multi peaked base ii) irregular rise iii) double/multi peaked peak iv) irregular decline ι ν) exaggerated gradual decline vi) sustained amplitude reduction 13% 0% 0% 0% 0% 86,9% 26% 4,3% 4,3% 4,3% 0% 0% 16,8% 1,9% 18,8% ' 8,9% 36,6% 43,5% 22,2% 3,7% 42,8% 40,5% 33,5% 31,7% 14,2% 2,8% 25,7% 14,2% 40% 34,2% 16% 8% 28% 34% 28% 30% c) shimmer 8,7% 4,3% 58,4% 59,2% 62,8% 56% d) fx i) jitter ii) abnormal fx drops 13% 8,7% 4,3% 0% 44,5% 27,7% 43,6% 26,1% 40% 28% 42% 25% 1 e) vocal fry 0% 0% 22,7% 23,8% 17,1% 24% die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 ingrid meyers and denise andersoi the most noticeable change for all three voice samples can be seen in the amplitude of the wave forms. table 4 indicates the change in average amplitude preand post-therapeutically. table 4 changes in average amplitude of waveforms preand post-therapeutically. prolonged /i:/ reading spontaneous speech pre post pre post pre post average amplitude in mm. 10mm 25mm 10mm 16mm 13mm 17mm b) fiberoptic examination: normal, symmetrical vocal folds were evident at rest and during deep breathing preand post-therapeutically. both the true and false vocal folds remained abducted. figures la and b indicate the physiological alteration of the vocal folds for the prolonged vowel preand post-therapeutically. •il» figure la figure lb figure la & lb. the physiological alteration of the vocal folds 'during the prolonged ihl pre(a) and post(b) therapeutically. figure la reveals that pre-therapeutically the vocal folds were tightly adducted with the false folds approximating one another in an asymmetrical fashion. the left fold was tensed, had more bulk and was more adducted than the right fold. figure lb shows that posttherapeutically the true cords were not tightly adducted and a chink was visible along their inner border. the false folds approximated but were symmetrical. on subjective evaluation, the vowel in fig la was perceived as having a tight, strained hoarse quality, while in fig. lb, it was perceived as having a breaking quality. figure 2b w ψ i / figure 2a figures 2a & 2b. the vocal folds during the production of the word /i2wd/ pre and post therapeutically respectively. figures 2a and b reveal an alteration in the physiology of the vocal folds for spontaneous speech preand post-therapeutically. in fig. 2a pre-therapeutically, total arrest of the glottis was apparent, where the false folds were tightly adducted iri the midline, and obscured the view of the true vocal folds. tension was evident posteriorly as adjustments in the arytenoid cartilages were made. subjectively, a tight squeezed, laryngealized quality was evident. posttherapeutically, less tension in the glottis was present. the true cords were not tightly adducted and a visible chink was present between the cords. while the false folds approximated they did not adduct. this was perceived as having a laryngealized quality on subjective evaluation. finally, table 5 summarises the results obtained from both the subjective and objective evaluations after the therapy period. table 5 overall summary of preand post-therapeutic measures for all three voice samples. subjective evaluation laryngograph tracings fiberoptic examination / reading + + spontaneous speech + + + + + prolonged /i:/ + + + + + + key: -r = slight improvement + + = improvement the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a stuttering therapy programme with spastic dysphonia a single discussion the results of the study indicated some improvements on all three voice samples for both subjective and objective measures posttherapeutically. the improvement was most evident in a reduction in adductor spasms, strained strangled phonation and overall vocal tension. subjectively, the voice was judged as less tense, and a reduction in %ss was evident on the reading and speech samples. the reduction in exaggerated gradual decline and increase in wave form amplitude on the laryngographic tracing seemed to indicate less tension and freer vibration of the vocal cords (traill 1984). the fiberoptic examination revealed a chink between the true folds which were not adducted as tightly as they had been previously. izdebski, dedo, shipp and flower (1981) write that the characteristic identified as overpressure (i.e. strain) is typical of spastic dysphonia, and thus the reduction in strain observed in this study could be considered a positive index of treatment outcome. lerman (1980) found that in spastic dysphonia, the over adducted vocal folds impeded the airflow, thus disrupting the delicate balance between airflow and glottic resistance which is necessary to produce acceptable phonation. the reduced rate of speech inherent in this therapy programme seemed to induce voluntary control over the adductor laryngeal muscles which facilitated the co-ordination of the muscles for phonation. sustained phonation and easy initiation of phonation were found to eliminate the tight adduction in the cords and the effortful forced phonation in this study. adams (1975) and schwartz (1976) feel prolonged speech facilitates glottal vibration by integrating sub-glottal air pressure, glottal resistance, supraglottal pressure, correct timing, smooth initiation and maintenance of airflow. stuttering occurred when this integration was disturbed. this imbalance was observed in the present study which further lends support to the similarity in vocal fold physiology of stuttering and spastic dysphonia, and to the applicability of a 'fluency based' programme with an adductor spastic dysphonia client. the study also showed that behaviour therapy can be applied to consciously manipulate vocal cord behaviour, thus supporting the views held by boone (1977), and mowrer and case (1982). a rough laryngeal tone was still noted on the post-therapy subjective evaluation for reading and spontaneous speech. this was confirmed by the laryngographic tracings and the presence of false fold approximation on the fiberoptic examination. boone (1977) writes that a rough vocal quality is caused by aperiodic vibration, and ventricular phonation may be causjed by the ventricular folds lying in close contact with the superior surfaces of the true cords. traill (1984) feels that the interference of the false folds above alters their tension and interferes with their movement. these perceptive characteristics showed up as irregular lx waveforms on the laryngographic tracings, where shimmer, jitter and irregular peaks and declines were still observed post-therapeutically. wechsler (1977) found that shimmer, jitter and irregular fold vibration could occur in normal speakers but horii (1980) found these features were more extensive in pathological speakers. the high percentage of the above three features on the laryngographs of this subject, indicated some pathology to be remaining in the voice. the writers contend, that while the 'fluency-based' programme was effective in altering the mode of laryngeal vibration, it did not sufficiently reduce the contribution of the false folds during phonation which resulted in the harsh ventricular vocal quality. since the technique employed in this study has been effective in modifying both true and false fold activity in stutterers, perhaps further practice is needed with the techniques in spastic dysphonia. vocal tremor was still evident post-therapeutically on the subjective assessment of all three voice samples. the writers speculate die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 case study 35 that irregular waveshapes, particularly the irregular base or rise in the laryngographic tracings, may be related to the tremulous vocal quality. the maintenance of the tremor in this subject is interesting since mccall (1975) noted that tremor in the voice of stutterers decreased when the mode of phonation was altered to induce fluent speech. the writers feel that the laryngeal adjustments necessary for maintaining continuous phonation might have caused the tremor, due to the novel mode of phonation to which the larynx had been subjected. perhaps further practice with the therapy technique might have reduced the amount of vocal tremor. an alteration in modal pitch occurred on the post-therapeutic measures. pre-therapeutically, a drop in the fx tracing of the laryngograph coincided with a tight, strained voice and exaggerated gradual abduction of the vocal cords. aronson et al. (1968) found that a low modal pitch coincided with hyperadducted vocal cords in more severe forms of spastic dysphonia. these features were more prevalent in the subject prior to therapy and his voice disorder was judged as being more severe. post-therapeutically, a reduction in the percentage of fx drops and exaggerated gradual decline on the laryngographic analysis were evident. a decrease in %ss and overall vocal severity was noted on subjective evaluation. thus, the subject's symptomatology confirmed the findings of aronson et al. (1968) that in milder forms of spastic dysphonia, fleeting moments of strained harshness are evident with the pitch not being severely affected. the results of this study indicate some discrepancy across the different voice samples, as well as in the subjective and objective evaluations. the prolonged vowel showed a greater improvement than the reading and speech samples which is an interesting finding, since studies on spastic dysphonia have indicated that the voice deteriorates in the contextual, communicative functions of speech (boone 1977; aronson 1980). in addition, studies have shown that the voice deteriorates when voiced voiceless transitions need to be made (dedo and shipp 1980). various factors could have been related to the discrepancy between the subjective and laryngographic results for reading and spontaneous speech. progress in therapy was slow and the phase of transfer had not yet been reached. boone (1977) maintains that with spastic dysphonia, much time must be spent in working on environmental factors to allow the patient to phonate in a normal, relaxed way in various environmental situations. the discrepancy on the reading and speaking analysis may have been the result of the subject's difficulty in maintaining the continuous phonation on reading throughout the therapy programme. the phase of mow-up included in the stutter-free speech programme has been found to be essential with stutterers (shames and florance 1980). regression after treatment might occur and therefore the need for longer treatment to maintain fluency in stutterers has been emphasized (perkins et al 1974; shames and florance 1980). wechsler (1977) found that in voice disorders, when the larynx appeared to have improved, but no improvement on lx waveforms was noted, further treatment and long-term follow-up was indicated lest relapse occurred. the findings of this study lend support to wechsler (1977) since improvement was evident subjectively, but little change was determined on the laryngographic waveforms. boone (1977) feels that long-term follow-up procedures are essential in spastic dysphonia which has such a high rate of symptom relapse. conclusion a 'fluency-based' therapy programme conventionally used for stutterers was found to be effective in altering the vocal behaviours of r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 ingrid meyers and denise anderso a spastic dysphonic client. the writers feel that the therapy resulted in a reduction of vocal fold hyperadduction. this is in agreement with perkins (1971) as cited by reed (1980) stating that regardless of etiology, therapy for spastic dysphonia could be based on a functional analysis of vocal fold behaviour, where the knowledge of laryngeal function is used to achieve a more efficient voice. acknowledgements the writers wish to thank prof. a. traill, department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, for his assistance, guidance and time in conducting the objective measures. references adams, m.r. vocal tract dynamics in fluency and stuttering. in vocal tract dynamics and dysfluency. webster, l.m. and furst, l.c. (eds.), speech and hearing institute, new york, 1975. aronson, a.e. clinical voice disorders: an interdisciplinary approach, thieme. stratton, new york, 1980. aronson, a.e., brown, j.r., litin, e.m., and pearson, j.s. spastic dysphonia i: voice neurologic and psychiatric aspects. j. speech. hear. disord., 33, 203-218, 1968. boone, d.r. the voice and voice therapy, 2nd. ed., prentice hall, new jersey, 1977. chapey, r. and salzberg, d. the speech clinician's use of fiberoptics in indirect laryngoscopy. j. comm. disord., 14, 87-90, 1981. dedo, h.h. and shipp, t.s. spastic dysphonia: a surgical and voice therapy treatment programme. college hill press, houston, 1980. fairbanks, g. voice and articulation drillbook. harper and brothers, new york, 1940. hirano, m. clinical examination of voice. springer verlag, new york, 1981. horii, y. vocal shimmer in sustained phonation. j. speech. hear. res. 23, 202-209, 1980. ingham, r.j. and andrews, g. behaviour therapy and stuttering: a review. j. speech. hear. disord., 38, 405-441, 1973. izdebski, k.: dedo, h.h.; shipp, m.d. and flower, m. postoperative and follow-up studies of spastic dysphonia patients treated by recurrent laiyngeal nerve section. otolaryngol. head. neck surg. 89, 96-101, 1981. kelman, a.w. vibratoiy patterns of the vocal folds. folia phoniat. 33 (2), 73-100, 1981. lerman, j.w. disorders of phonation and their management. ear, nose and throat j. 59, 62-72, 1980. luchsinger, r. and arnold, g.e. voice speech language. wordsworth publishing co., california, 1965. mccall, g.n. spasmodic dysphonia and the stuttering block. in vocal tract dynamics and dysfluency webster, l.m. and furst, l.c. (eds). speech and hearing institute, new york, 1975. miller, s. airflow therapy programmes. facts and/or fancy. j. fluency dis. 7, 187-202, 1982. mowrer, d.e. and case, j.l. clinical management of speech disorders. aspen systems corporation u.s.a. 1982. perkins, w.h.: rudas, j.; johnson, l.; michael, w.g. and curlee, r.f. replacement of stuttering with clinical effectiveness iii. j. speech, hear. disord., 39, 416-428, 1974. reed, g.g. voice therapy: a need for research. j. speech hear disord., 45 (2), 157-171, 1980. "reich, a. and till, j. phonatory and manual reaction time of women with idiopathic spasmodic dysphonia. j. speech hear. disord., 26 (1), 10-17, 1983. sal amy, j.n. and sessions, r.b. spastic dysphonia. j. fluency dis 5, 281-290, 1980. schwartz, m. stuttering solved. j.b. lipincott, philadelphia, new york, 1976. shames, g.h. and florance, c.l. stutter-free speech. charles e. merrill, columbus, ohio, 1980. traill, a. personal communication. department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, 1984. wechsler, e. a laiyngographic study of voice disorders. brit. j. dis. comm. 12, 19-22, 1977. wilson, d.k.: voice problems of children 2nd ed. williams and wilkins, baltimore, 1979. wolk, l. spastic dysphonia: a case report s.a.j. commun. disord., 2, 3-18, 1980. appendix 1 reading and spontaneous speech scale parameter 1) laryngeal tension normal 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 extremely tense 2) laryngeal tone normal 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 exceptionally harsh/hoarse 3) tremor in contextual speech none 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 exceptional 4) voice stoppages none 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 excessive 5) intermittent strain-strangle none 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 excessive 6) constant strain-strangle none 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 excessive 7) loudness normal 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 exceptionally loud/soft 8) pitch normal 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 exceptionally high/low 9) vocal inflections normal 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 monotonous 10) pitch breaks to higher pitches none 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 excessive 11) rate normal 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 exceptionally fast/slow, 12) overall vocal efficiency ' efficient 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 inefficient 13) naturalness • natural 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 unnatural i i i appendix 2 ! summary of categories analyzed on laryngographic analysis adapted from'boone (1977), horii (1980) and traill (1984). lx waveform provides information on vocal fold vibratory a) cycle. a) regularity maintenance of a stable vibratory pattern. b) waveshape provides a basis for observing the nature of/ contact and separation time of the vocal folds. c) shimmer changes in adjacent wave amplitudes. fx waveform vocal pitch. a) jitter cycle to cycle variations in frequency. cor' responds with shimmer on lx. shimmer and jitter b) correlate with rough/hoarse voice. b) abnormal fx drops exaggerated drops in fx tracing. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) philips hearing aid services a d i v i s i o n of s . a . p h i l i p s (pty) ltd. hearing amplaid audiometers earmark systems philips hearing aid services head office 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street, p.o. box 3069, johannesburg. t i suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 9 die invloed van otitis media op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme lidia olivier, b.log (pretoria) brenda louw, d.phil (pretoria) rene hugo, d.phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria opsommjng otitis media word in die resente literatuur in verband gebring met spraaken taalprobleme, gedragsprobleme en leerprobleme. beperkte navorsing is egter tot op hede uitgevoer om die invloed wat otitis media op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens uitoefen, te bepaal. die doel met hierdie studie is om te bepaal of otitis media wel 'n invloed op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme uitoefen. dertien persone wat gediagnoseer is as kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme, is as proefpersone geselekteer. hulle is in twee groepe ingedeel, naamlik agt proefpersone met 'n geskiedenis van otitis media (groep a) en vyf proefpersone sonder 'n geskiedenis van otitis media (groep b). 'n toetsbattery is saamgestel vir die evaluering van die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens en is op die proefpersone uitgevoer. in die geval van groep a was die resultate afwykend in elke subtoets in die sentrale toetsbattery wat op 'n moontlike breinstambetrokkendheid dui. groep β het normale resultate in bykans al die toetse verkry. die resultate dui daar op dat ehroniese otitis media wel 'n invloed op die sen trale ouditiewe vermoens van die proefpersone het en wel in terme van onvoldoende breinstamfunksionering. implikasies vir navorsing en behandeling is bespreek. abstract in recent literature otitis media is linked to speech and language problems, behavioural problems and learning disabilities. until recently limited research has been done in order to determine the influence of otitis media on the central auditory abilities. the aim of this study was to determine whether otitis media has an influence on the central auditory abilities of children with sensory integration problems. thirteen persons diagnosed as children with sensory integration problems were selected as subjects. theg were divided into two groups, namelg eight subjects with a history of otitis media (group a ) and five subjects without a history of otitis media (group b). a test battery was compiled for evaluation of the central auditory abilities and was used on the subjects. in terms of group a deviating results were obtained on every subtest of the central auditory test batiery which indicated a possible brainstem involvement. group β obtained normal results in more or less all the ίtests. the results therefore suggest that chronic otitis media has an influence on the central auditory abilities, i.e. in terms of insufficient brainstem functioning. implications for research and treatment were discussed. otitis media is tans 'n populere navorsingsonderwerp, hoofsaaklik as gevolg van die jomvattende invloed wat dit op die kind kan uitoefen (gottlieb, zinkus & thompson, 1979). dwarsdeur die literatuur word otitis media in verband met spraaken taalprobleme, gedragsprobleme en leerprobleme gebring (gottlieb et al. 1979). alhoewel otitis media ook in verband gebring word met die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens, is daar tot op hede nog beperkte navorsing in hierdie verband uitgevoer. in die lig daarvan dat die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens 'n belangrike rol in sensoriese integrasie speel, en deur 'n verband tussen otitis media en sentrale ouditiewe prosessering te bepaal, kan nuwe inligting, oor veral die invloed van otitis media, maar ook die rol van die oudioloog by die kind met sensoriese integrasieprobleme, beskikbaar gestel word. otitis media is 'n kindersiekte met 'n baie hoe voorkoms, veral by kinders van sesjaar en jonger (mcshane & mitchell, 1979). teenstrydighede ten opsigte van die invloed van otitis media op kinders kom egter i in die literatuur voor en navorsingsbevindinge kan hoofsaaklik in twee groepe verdeel word die suid-afrikaanse tdskrif vir kommunikasieafins, vol. 37 1990 (paradise, 1983). die eerste groep navorsers toon aan dat kinders wat wel 'n geskiedenis van otitis media het, maar van wie die gehoo'r normaal was tydens toetsing, geen of slegs 'n geringe agterstand toon in terme van sintaksis, taalbegrip, leesvermoe en woordeskat (owrid, 1970). die tweede groep navorsers het bevind dat otitis media wel 'n negatiewe invloed op taalontwikkeling, veral in terme van taalinhoud en -vorm het (blager, 1982; sak & rubin, 1982). volgens paradise (1983) kan hierdie geskilpunt in die literatuur verklaar word uit leemtes in die navorsing. diagnose van otitis media in vroeere lewensjare is ten eerste nie baie betroubaar nie. tydens toetsing vertoon sommige proefpersone otitis media en kan daar dus nie akkurate inligting met betrekking tot hulle ontwikkeling bekom word nie. slegs beperkte steekproewe is gewoonlik gebruik en die betroubaarheid van die studies word in baie gevalle betwyfel (paradise, 1983). as daar egter in gedagte gehou word dat otitis media soms gepaard gaan met 'n geringe konduktiewe gehoorverlies, is dit 'n logiese afleiding dat dit wel 'n invloed op die normale όπιο savsg 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 0 lidia olivier, brenda louw, rene hugo wikkelingsverloop van die kind moet uitoefen. die negatiewe sekondere gevolge van otitis media kan verklaar word op grond van die wisselende graad van ouditiewe deprivasie wat ondervind word. studies het aangetoon dat die eerste vyf lewensjare veral belangrik is vir breinmaturasie (webster, 1983). die eerste vier lewensjare is ook die kritieke periode van taalaanleer (rubin, 1984). dit is in hierdie tyd, maar veral gedurende die eerste twee lewensjare dat otitis media by kinders voorkom (northern & downs, 1984). gehoor is noodsaaklik vir die ontwikkeling van die ouditiewe bane in die perifere en sentrale ouditiewe sisteem. 'n fluktuerende konduktiewe verlies as gevolg van otitis media kan 'n negatiewe invloed op die ontwikkeling van die ouditiewe funksie, sowel as op kognitiewe en kommunikasievaardighede uitoefen (hornsby, 1984). die ontwikkeling van ouditiewe prosessering word ook nadelig bei'nvloed deur otitis media, aangesien die ouditiewe boodskappe nie die sentrale senuweestelsel akkuraat bereik nie (zinkus, 1982). afwykings in sentrale prosessering, versteuring in ouditiefvisuele integrasie, leesprobleme en ouditiewe persepsieprobleme word dus in verband gebring met die herhaaldelike.voorkoms van otitis media, (gottlieb, 1979; zinkus, 1982). webster (1983) is ook eens dat 'n chroniese konduktiewe gehoorverlies in die vroee lewensjare tot 'n permanente sentrale ouditiewe afwyking kan lei. otitis media kan dus 'n invloed op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van kinders uitoefen. die ouditiewe sisteem, en in die besonder die sentrale ouditiewe sisteem, is veral in die geval van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme van belang as 'n sensoriese modaliteit vir die persepsie van ouditiewe invoer. normaalweg word die sensoriese inligting, afkomstig van die ouditiewe sisteem, met die sensoriese invoer van ander sensoriese modaliteite, naamlik die vestibulere sisteem, taktiele sisteem, propriosepsie en die visuele sisteem gei'ntegreer sodat daar toepaslik daarop gereageer kan word. die interpretasie en integrasie, asook toepaslike reaksie op sensoriese stimuli, staan bekend as sensoriese integrasie. die proses word deur die sentrale senuweestelsel teweeggebring deurdat 'n massa sensoriese inligting gefiltreer, georganiseer en gei'ntegreer word, sodat dit gebruik kan word in die ontwikkeling en uitvoering van die brein se funksies (ayres, 1973). 'n wanfunksie ten opsigte van sensoriese integrasie beteken dat die vloei van sensoriese inligting deur middel van die sensoriese modaliteite nie voldoende geprosesseer en georganiseer word nie. sodoende kan die individu nie presiese inligting oor homself en sy wereld beleef nie (ayres, 1973). dit strem die individu in die ontwikkeling van hoer breinfunksies en -kan, onder andere, tot die volgende simptome lei: hiperaktiwiteit; afleibaarheid; gedragsprobleme; spierwankoordiriasie; taalen spraakprobleme, asook leerprobleme. navorsing toon egter dat sensoriese integrasieprobleme tot leerprobleme kan lei en in baie gevalle nou verwant is aan leerprobleme (ayres, 1973). baie van die simptome van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme stem ooreen met die simptome van leergestremde kinders, bv. ouditiewe prosesseringsprobleme, asook verskeie uitvalle ten opsigte van taalfunksies (ayres, 1973). in die geval van kinders met leerprobleme word hierdie probleme veral in verband gebring met die simptome en gevolge van otitis media. zinkus (1982) dui ook aan dat kinders met leerprobleme 'n hoer voorkoms van otitis media (25 36%) as normale kinders (13 17%) toon, veral as hulle probleme verwant is aan ouditiewe vaardighede. aangesien otitis media wel 'n invloed op kinders met leerprobleme het, en wel veral in terme van hulle sentrale ouditiewe vermoens, kan daarverwag word dat otitis media ook 'n invloed kan he op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme. die bepaling van die verband tussen otitis media en die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens kan nuwe lig op die etiologie en simptomatologie van sensoriese intregrasieprobleme asook op die implikasies van otitis media werp. insluiting van die oudioloog in die span wat die kind met sensoriese integrasieprobleme hanteer, kan tot voorkoming van die nadelige invloed van otitis media op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens, en gevolglik die algehele funksionering van die kind, lei. metode doel die doel van die studie is om die invloed van otitis media op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme te bepaal deur: — die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van twee groepe kindersmet sensoriese integrasieprobleme, naamlik die met en die sonder 'n geskiedenis van otitis media te peil — intergroepsooreenkomste en -verskille tussen die twee eksperimentele groepe te bepaal. — intragroepsooreenkomste en -verskille tussen die proefpersone in elke eksperimentele groep te bepaal. navorsingsontwerp 'n beskrywende navorsingsplan is gebruik aangesien dit intra en intergroepsvergelyking, waarvolgens die gestelde navorsingsvrae beantwoord kan word, moontlik maak (steyn et al. 1984). ι protfpersoonseleksie \ i die proefpersone het dertien kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme ingesluit. die dertien proefpersone is met behulp van 'n vraelys geselekteer uit die totale aantal afijikaanssprekende kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme wat terapie ontvang by 'n privaat arbeidsen spraakterapiekliniek. agt kinders met 'n geskiedenis van otitis media (groep a) en vyf sonder 'n geskiedenis daarvan (groep b) is op 'n eenvoudige ewekansige wyse geselekteer. die proefpersone moes oor normale perifere gehoor beskik en tussen die ouderdomme 6 en 10,5 jaar wees. tabel 1 verskaf 'n opsommende weergawe van die kenmerke van die proefpersone. ' / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) invloed van otitis media op sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme tabel 1: algemene beeld van proefpersone w a t in hierdie ondersoek ingesluit is 41 * normale timpanogram dit wil se normale "compliance" en middeloordruk. + geen sluiting verkry. het reeds 22 maal buisies in beide ore gekry. eksperimentele groep proefpersoon geslag ouderdom gemiddelde gehoordrempel timpanogram groep a l r kinders met sensoriese 1 v 8j omnde 0 db 0 db normale tipe a* integrasieprobleme met 2 μ 7j 5mnde 13,3 db 10 db normale tipe a 'n geskiedenis van 3 μ 9j 9mnde 5 db 3,3 db normale tipe a otitis media 4 μ 6j 4mnde 6,6 db 6,6 db normale tipe a 5 μ 6j 4mnde 10 db 11,6 db geen sluiting"1" 6 v 6j 1 omnde 0 db 0 db normale tipe a 7 μ 7j 2mnde 3,3 db 3,3 db normale tipe a 8 v 7j l m n d 0 db 0 db normale tipe a groep β kinders met sensoriese 9 μ 9j 6mnde 0 db 3,3 db normale tipe a integrasieprobleme 10 v 7j 5mnde 8,3 db ' 3,3 db normale tipe a sonder 'n geskiedenis 11 μ 6j 6mnde 0 db 0 db normale tipe a van otitis media 12 v 8j l l m n d e 0 db 0 db normale tipe a 13 μ loj 6mnde 8,3 db 8,3 db normale tipe a prosedure data-insameliny 'n volledige gehoorevaluasie is op elke proefpersoon uitgevoer. vir hierdie doel is 'n gsi-10 oudiometer en 33 middle ear analizer gebruik asook telephonies tdh-50 oorfone met mx41/ar kussings. die oudiometer is op 1989-01-10 gekalitabel 2: sentrale ouditiewe sisteem toetsbattery breer en voldoen aan die vereistes van sabs 0154-1979. die eksperimentele toetsbattery was daarop gemik om die sentrale ouditiewe sisteem te evalueer en word in tabel 2 verskaf. toets motivering vir insluiting by toetsbattery a perifere gehoorevaluasie: 1 suiwertoontoetsing 2 spraakontvangsdrempel 3' spraakdiskriminasie ill stilte 4 timpanometrie β toetse vir sentrale ouditiewe prosessering 5 akoestiese refleksdrempelbepaling 6 sintetiese sinsidentifikasie (ssi) ssi-pi ssi-ikb (ipsilaterale kompeterende boodskap 7 maskeringsvlakverskiltoets (mw-toets) met suiwertone as stimulus met spondeewoorde as stimulus 8 simultaneous binaural median-plane localization test (sbmpl-test) i 1 normale perifere gehoor is 'n voorvereiste vir die uitvoering van sentrale ouditiewe toetse (musiek, 1985). 2 word as basis gebruik waarvolgens die intensiteit vir aanbieding van ander toetsstimuli bepaal is. 3 deur middel van die bepaling van spraakdiskriminasie in stilte by 4-5 intensiteite is die fg-pi-funksies (fonetiesgebalanseerde woordelysprestasie-intensiteitsfunksie) bepaal, om dit te kan vergelyk met die ssl-pi-funksie (sintetiese sinsidentifikasieprestasie-intensiteitsfunksie) om tot differensiaaldiagnose by te dra. 4 'n objektiewe meting van middeloorfunksie om die moontlikheid van middeloor-betrokkenheid uit te skakel. 5 vergelyking van ipsilaterale en kontralaterale reflekse verskaf waardevolle inligting vir moontlike identifikasie van breinstamletsels (hall, 1985). 6 ssi-pi versus fg-pi verskaf inligting vir 'n differensiaaldiagnose tussen kogleare retro-kogleare en sentrale ouditiewe senuweestelselletsels (jerger & hayes, 1977 in rintelman, 1985). differensieer verder tussen sentrale letsels (breinstam versus temporale lobletsels). 7 verskaf inligting oor laer breinstamfunksionering (noffsinger et al. 1985). 8 verskaf inligting oor algemene breinstamfunksionering. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommuniksieawykins, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 2 lidia olivier, brenda louw, rene hugo dat a-analise response van die proefpersone is deurentyd op toetsvorms aangeteken. die data is daarna in terme van die volgende kriteria beoordeel: akoestiese reflekse die akoestiese reflekse is beoordeel in terme van: normaal, verhoog, afwesig en verlaag. indien die akoestiese refleks (ipsilateraal en kontralateraal) tussen 80-100 dbsp voorgekom het, is dit as normaal beskou (fria et al. 1985 en metz, 1946 in northern et al. 1985). enige refleks wat hoer as loodb intree, is as 'n verhoogde refleks beskou en enige refleks laer as 60db as 'n verlaagde refleks. sintetiese-sinsidentifikasietoets die ssi-pi-funksie versus die fg-pi-funksie is beoordeel in die lig van jerger & hayes se kriteria (rintelman, 1985). die resultate van die ssi-ikb en ssi-kkb is volgens die kriteria van jerger & jerger (1975) soos aangedui deur musiek en pinheiro (1975) beoordeel. figuur 1: staafkaart van akoestiese reflekse in figuur 1 word die ipsilaterale en kontralaterale refleksresultate uiteengesit. dit blyk duidelik uit die voorstelling dat groep a en β normale ipsilateral^ reflekse vertoon het. maskeringsvlakverskiltoets die resultate van die maskeringsvlakverskiltoets is in terme van afwykend en normaal beoordeel. die normale maskeringsvlakverskilwaarde by 500hz, dit wil se, die verskil in drempelwaardes met aanbieding van die toetsstimuli in (0/0)en uit (0/180)-fase is 10-15db (noffsinger et al. 1985). 'n vermindering in die maskeringsvlakverskilwaarde, dit wil se, minder as lodb word as afwykende resultate beskou. simultaneous binaural median-plane localization test twee beoordelings is gemaak, naamlik normaal of afwykend. die vermoe om die klank waar te neem met .'n interourale intensiteitsverskil van o-lodb is beskou as normale resultate. onvermoe om die klank sentraal waar te neem of sentrale waarneming van die klank met groot interourale-intensiteitsverskille, dit wil se, groter as 10-15db is as afwykende resultate beskou (jerger, 1960). resultate resultate word bespreek in dieselfde volgorde as wat die toetsbattery uitgevoer is. akoestiese reflekse in terme van die kontralaterale reflekse het groep a normale, verhoogde en afwesige kontralaterale reflekse vertoon. in geval van groep β het normale-kontralaterale reflekspatrone voorgekom. / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 akoestiese reflekse ipsilaterale & kontralaterale reflekse normaal verhooq ι verlaaq afwesiq i pel met o.m i pel sonder o.m kontra met o.m kontra sonder o.m 100% 100% 37.5% 100% 0% 0% 25% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 0% 37.5% 0% η ipsi met o.m ϋ § 1 ipsi sonder o.m ted kontra met o.m ^ m kontra sonder o.m figuur 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) invloed van otitis media op sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme 4 3 sintetiese sinsidentifikasietoets (ssi) in hierdie geval is twee metings gemaak, naamlik: 'n vergelyking tussen die fg-pi-funksie en die ssi-pifunksie — vergelyking van ssi resultate in die teenwoordigheid van 'n ipsilaterale kompeterende boodskap asook 'n kontralaterale kompeterende boodskap. fg-pi versus -pi die gemiddelde van die resultate word aangetoon in figure 2a en 2b. 10 i s 2 0 30 intensiteit in db -fg-pi-funksie ssi-pi-funksie figuur 2(a): ssi-pi-fi»nksie versus fg-pi-funksie: groep a , db 70 i 15 20 25 intensiteit in db -fg-pi-funksie -ssi-pi-funksie figuur 2(b): ssi-pi-funksie versus fg-pi-funksie: groep β figuur 2(a) toon dat die proefpersone in groep a se resultate op die fg-pi-funksie binne normale perke was. die spraakdiskriminasievermoens van die proefpersone het telkens 100% gebly met 'n verhoging in intensiteit nadat die persentasie maksimum korrekte spraakdiskriminasie bereik is. die ssi-pifunksie is heelwat swakker en verloop onder die fg-pi-funksie. dit vertoon ook telkens 'n sogenaamde "roll-over" (jerger en hayes, 1977 in tintelman, 1985). groep β vertoon afwykende resultate (figuur 2(b)) van die ssipi-funksie in die sin dat die ssi-pi-funksie onder die fg-pifunksie verloop en telkens 'n sogenaamde "roll-over" vertoon (jerger & hayes, 1977 in rintelman, 1985). hierdie resultate is egter in 'n mindere mate afwykend in vergelyking met groep a se resultate. in geval van groep β was die fg-pi-funksie ook binne normale perke. / / / 2 0 1 0 s/r verhouding sonder o.m. met o.m. figuur 3(a): resultate van ssi-ikb (links) 80% 20% 2 0 1 0 s/r verhouding sonder o.m. m e t o.m. figuur 3(b): resultate van ssi-ikb (regs) uit hierdie figure is dit duidelik dat beide groep a en β afwykende resultate vertoon het. groep a se resultate is egter in 'n meerdere mate afwykend in vergelyking met die van groep b. maskeringsvlakverskiltoets figuur 4 is 'n voorstelling van die resultate wat in die maskeringvlakverskiltoets vir suiwertone en spraak verkry is. resultate maskeringsvlakverskiltoets verskil in drempelwaardes 20 1 6 suiwer tone spondee-woorde proefpersone 1 spondee-woorde ssi-pi versus ssi-kkb die resultate van die ssi-ikb word deur die volgende figure voorgestel. figuur 4: resultate van die maskeringsvlakverskiltoets die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykinfls, vol. 37, lij^,^. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 4 lidia olivier, brenda louw, rene hugo dit blyk dat daar in terme van beide die suiwertoonen spraakstimuli afwykende resultate by al die proefpersone in groep a verkry is. hulle resultate is afwykend aangesien die verskil in responsdrempels wat verkry is by die aanleiding van die toetsstimuli en die kompeterende boodskap in fase (0°/0°) en uit fase (0°/180°) lodb en minder was. normale response is by groep β verkry, dit wil se die verskil in responsdrempels met die aanbieding van die suiwertoonen spraakstimuli en die kompeterende boodskap in en uit fase was lodb en meer. simultaneous binaural median-plane localization test figuur 5 is 'n voorstelling van die resultate wat verkry is. sbmpl-resultate figuur 5: resultate van die sbmpl-toets uit hierdie voorstelling blyk dit dat die proefpersone van groep a se resultate in 'n groot mate van die van groep β verskil. groep a het meestal 'n onvermoe getoon om die klank sentraal waar te neem of dit is wel sentraal waargeneem met groot interourale intensiteitsverskille. groep β se resultate was heeltemal normaal. bespreking van resultate dit wil voorkom asof die eksperimentele vraag of herhaaldelike otitis media in die vroee lewensjare 'n invloed op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoerjf van persone met sensoriese integrasieprobleme uitoefen, deur die verkree resultate beantwoord is. uit die resultate blyk dit duidelik dat daar in die geval van groep a, die proefpersone met 'n geskiedenis van otitis media, oor die algemeen swakker resultate in die sentrale ouditiewe toetsbattery gelewer het in vergelyking met groep β wat bestaan het uit proefpersone sonder 'n geskiedenis van otitis media. in terme van die akoestiese refleksmeting dui verskeie outeurs (northern et al. 1985; griessen & rasmussen 1970 in hall, 1985) aan dat afwesige of verhoogde kontralaterale reflekse met gepaardgaande normale ipsilaterale reflekse 'n aanduiding van breinstampatologie is. dit blyk dus dat die proefpersone in groep a se resultate 'n aanduiding van moontlike breinstampatologie kan wees. volgens northern et al. (1985) kan dit verklaar word op grond van die teenwoordigheid van 'n letsel of wanfunksionering in die area waar die breinstambane kruis. die ongekruisde bane bly intakt en het dus normale ipsilaterale reflekse tot gevolg. in die geval van die proefpersone in groep β (proefpersone sonder 'n geskiedenis van otitis media) was die resultate deurgaans normaal. die resultate van die akoestiese refleksmeting word deur die ssi-toetsbattery geverifieer. die resultate van die fg-pi-funksie versus die ssi-pi-funksie dui daarop dat die fg-pi-funksie se resultate in geval van die proefpersone in beide groep a en β binne normale perke geval het. die ssi-pi-funksie het in die geval van die proefpersone in groep a, asook in die van groep b, onder die fg-pi-funksie verloop en 'n sogenaamde "roll-over" vertoon. die proefpersone in groep a se prestasie in die ssi-pifunksie was egter heel wat swakker as die van die proefpersone in groep b. jerger & hayes (1977) in rintelman (1985) het bevind dat, indien die ssi-pi-funksie onder die fg-pi-funksie verloop, dit 'n aanduiding van 'n sentrale afwyking in die kontralaterale oor is. in hierdie studie was die resultate afwykend in beide ore van al die proefpersone in groepe a en b. die teenwoordigheid van 'n sentrale betrokkenheid by die vergelyking van die ssi-pi-funksie en die fg-pi-funksie dui egter nie die spesifieke area van wanfunksionering aan nie. in die geval van die ssi-ikb versus ssi-kkb dui die resultate daarop dat die ssi-kkb in die geval van al die proefpersone binne normale perke was. met betrekking tot die ipsilaterale kompeterende boodskap (ssi-ikb) het al die proefpersone afwykende resultate vertoon. net soos in die geval van die fgpi-funksie versus die ssi-pi-funksie was die resultate van die proefpersone in groep a in 'n meerdere mate afwykend in vergelyking met die van die proefpersone in groep b: jerger (1981) het aangetoon dat in die geval van kinders met onvoldoende breinstamfunksionering die prestasie in die ssi-kkb binne normale perke val, terwyl die ssi-ikb afwykende resultate vertoon. in die verband verklaar jerger & jerger (1974): "for brainstem patients, the ssi procedure shows poor performance of icm and relatively good performance for ccm. the icm deficits are observed on the contralateral ear and ccm performance is within normal limits on both ears." (1974, p. 342). i wat interessant is, is dat die proefpersone in groep β met betrekking tot die ssi-toetsbattery swakker gepresteer het. toetsresultate met betrekking tot die res van die sentrale toetsbattery was binne normale perke. die swakker prestasie in die ssi-toetsbattery kan in die lig van die volgende verklaar word: kinders met normale gehoor leer taal aan eerstens deur die ouditiewe sisteem. lees is 'n sekondere taalvaardigheid wat afhanklik is van visueel-ouditiewe integrasie in die breinstam, soos gevind in studies van birch & belmont, kahn !&, birch & bartholomeus en doehring in 1976 (musiek & pinheiro, 1985). kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme ondervind probleme met visuele-ouditiewe integrasie. volgens ayres (1983) speel die breinstam 'n sentrale rol in normale sensoriese integrasie. die breinstam organiseer alle sensoriese informasie deur middel van inhibisie, fasilitasie en sintese om sodoende 'n volledige interpretasie van alle sensoriese stimuli te maak. dit help die brein om op een tipe sensoriese invoer te fokus, terwyl die res gei'nhibeer word. in die geval van die ssi-toetsbattery was nie net al die proefpersone se vermoe om die visuele stimuli met die ouditiewe stimuli te integreer, aangetas nie, maar ook die vermoe om die pf imere stimuli te kan isoleer deur middel van inhibering van moontlike waarneming van die kompeterende boodskap. laasgenoemde twee aspekte is volgens ayres (1983) kenmerkend van kinders met the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) invloed van otitis media op sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van kinders met sensoriese integrasieprobleme 4 5 sensoriese integrasieprobleme en dit kan dus 'n bydraende faktor wees tot die swakker prestasie in die ssi-toetsbattery in hierdie studie. laastens wat die ssi-.toetse betref, moet daarop gewys word dat die proefpersone in groep a se prestasie in die ssi-toetsbattery swakker was as die van die proefpersone in groep b. dit kan waarskynlik die gevolg wees van die herhaalde otitis media in die vroee lewensjare wat moontlik 'n invloed op breinstamfunksionering gehad het. die maskeringvlakverskiltoets bevestig ook die bevindinge tot dusver. die proefpersone in groep a se resultate was afwykend in geval van beide die suiwertoonen spraakstimuli in die sin dat die responsdrempels wat verkry is by die aanbieding van die toetsstimuli en kompeterende boodskap in fase (0°/ 0°) en uit fase (0°/180°), lodb en minder was. normale response deur die proefpersone in groep β is verkry, dit wil se die verskil in responsdrempels met die aanbieding van die suiwertoonen spraakstimuli en die kompeterende boodskap in en uit fase was lodb en meer. volgens verskeie outeurs soos sweetow & reddel (in willeford, 1985) hou die maskeringsvlakverskiltoets belowende resultate in indien 'n breinstamafwyking vermoed word tydens die toetsing van leergestremde kinders. die maskeringsvlakverskiltoets se resultate in hierdie studie bevestig ook die moontlikheid van 'n breinstamletsel of onvoldoende breinstamfunksionering. aangesien daar geen ander veranderlikes as otitis media was nie, kan daar 'n korrelasie tussen die voorkoms van otitis media en die afwykende resultate op die maskeringsvlakverskiltoets bepaal word. bogenoemde resultate word verder bevestig deur die sbmpltoetsresultate wat ook op 'n algemene breinstamdisfunksie dui. die proefpersone in groep a was nie in staat om die klank sentraal waar te neem nie; of die klank is sentraal waargeneem met groot interourale-intensiteitsverskille (meer as lodb). stephens (1976) het gevind dat persone met 'n breinstamletsel afwykende resultate in die sbmpl-toets toon. in die lig van die verkree resultate in die sentrale toetsbattery blyk dit duidelik dat daar 'njmoontlike breinstamletsel of wan..funksionering in die geval van die proefpersone in groep a voorkom. aangesien die proefpersone in groep β se resultate op die sentrale ouditiewe toetsbattery (met uitsluiting van die ssi-toetsbattery) binne normale perke was, ontstaan die vraag waarom die proefpersone in groep a dan beduidend swakker presteer het op die toetsbattery. aangesien otitis media die enigste veranderlike is wat die twee eksperimentele groepe onderskei, noodsaak dit 'n diepgaande ondersoek na die moontlike rol wat otitis media in die sentrale ouditiewe gehoorvermoens kan speel, veral met betrekking tot breinstamfunksionering. verskeie menings oor die rol van otitis media in die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van leergestremde kinders, word in die literatuur voorgehou. gottlieb et al. (1979) toon 'n duidelike verband aan tussen otitis media en die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens, asook die visuele integrasievermoens van leergestremde kinders. volgens zinkus (1982) toon verskeie leergestremde kinders sentrale ouditiewe probleme in bv reeksgeheue; diskriminasie en sluiting. dit is die gevolg van onvoldoende prosessering van ouditiewe invoer, alhoewel die kind se kognitiewe funksie intakt is. identifikasie, diskriminasie en organisasie wat die basis van die ouditiewe leerproses vorm, kan dus in die geval van hierdie kinders afwykend wees. johnson (1981) meen dat leergestremde kinders met sentrale ouditiewe prosesseringsprobleme ook hoerisiko-gevalle is vir otitis media. die fluktuerende gehoorverlies wat tydens periodes van otitis media voorkom, het dus moontlik 'n beplande invloed op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens van die leergestremde kind (zinkus, 1982). die fluktuerende gehoorverlies lei tot onvoldoende ouditiewe invoer tydens die vroee lewensjare. aangesien die vroee lewensjare die kritieke tyd is vir spraaken taalontwikkeling, asook vir maturasie van, onder andere, die ouditiewe sisteem, is dit logies om tot die gevolgtrekking te kom dat otitis media wel 'n invloed op die maturasie van die ouditiewe sisteem het. dit kan lei tot latere onvoldoende ouditiewe prosessering wat die grondslag van sentrale ouditiewe probleme vorm. die moontlike invloed wat otitis media in die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens kan speel, word verder toegelig deur diere-eksperimente. studies deur webster en webster (1977, in zinkus, 1982) toon dat 'n konduktiewe gehoorverlies tydens kritieke periodes van breinmaturasie tot morfologiese veranderinge in sekere neurone van die ouditiewe nuklei kan lei. volgens webster (1983) is die ouditiewe sisteem afhanklik van omgewingsinvoer en kan daar dus duidelik strukturele veranderinge plaasvind as gevolg van 'n chroniese konduktiewe gehoorverlies in die vroee lewensjare. in 'n studie met muise is ook aangetoon dat 'n herhaaldelike konduktiewe gehoorverlies tydens kritieke postnatale periodes tot anatomiese en fisiologiese veranderinge in die breinstam lei. volgens katz & wilde (1985) het verskeie navorsers ook tot soortgelyke gevolgtrekkings oor die invloed van otitis media op die sentrale ouditiewe sisteem gekom (holm & kunze, 1969; northern & downs, 1978; rubin & rapin, 1980). dit blyk dus duidelik uit die literatuur dat die herhaaldelike konduktiewe gehoorverlies wat tydens chroniese otitis media voorkom, 'n invloed op die maturasie van veral die breinstam en uiteindelik op die sentrale ouditiewe gehoorvermoens het. katz & wilde (1985) is dit ook eens dat die sogenaamde ouditiewe deprivasie-effek die mees logiese verklaring is vir die uiteindelike langtermyninvloed wat otitis media op die sentrale ouditiewe vermoens het. sensoriese deprivasie speel uiteindelik 'n bydraende rol in terme van afwykende vermoens in die organisering en strukturering van inligting (ayres, 1983). voldoende stimuli is beskikbaar, maar onvoldoende prosessering vind plaas, aangesien die struktuur wat noodsaaklik is vir optimale prosessering nie voldoende ontwikkel het nie as gevolg van sensoriese deprivasie tydens kritieke stadia van ontwikkeling. indien hierdie studie se resultate in verband met bogenoemde verklaring gebring word, kan die gevolgtrekking gemaak word dat die proefpersone in groep a se swakker prestasie in die sentrale toetsbattery moontlik te wyte is aan vroee ouditiewe deprivasie waf veroorsaak is deur otitis media. die sensoriese deprivasie kon gelei hettotonvoldoende maturasie van veral die breinstam soos gemanifesteer in die verkree resultate van die studie. die resultate van hierdie studie het dus nie net op die korrelasie van otitis media en die sentrale ouditiewe probleme van die proefpersone in groep agedui nie, maar veral op die korrel; sie tussen otitis media en die breinstamfunksionering van die proefpersone in groep a. volgens zinkus (1982) kan chroniese otitis media egter nie gesien word as die enigste oorsaak van alle sentrale ouditiewe probleme nie. die etiologie van sentrale probleme kan dikwels nie gedefinieer word nie. katz & wilde (1985) toon aan dat alhoewel tumors en ander sentrale letsels in kinders kan voorkom, sentrale ouditiewe toetse by kinders meestal uitgevoer word in die lig van moontlike ouditiewe prosesseringsof leerprobleme. die doel met die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafun/kint/s, vul. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 6 l i d i a o l i v i e r , b r e n d a l o u w , r e n e h u g o s e n t r a l e t o e t s i n g b y k i n d e r s is d u s n i e o m die l o k u s v a n ' n letsel te b e p a a l n i e , m a a r e e r d e r o m die f u n k s i o n e r i n g e n m a t u r a s i e v a n die v e r s k i l l e n d e d e l e v a n die s e n t r a l e o u d i t i e w e s i s t e e m te e v a l u e e r . i n die lig v a n h i e r d i e s t u d i e b e v e s t i g die v e r k r e e r e s u l t a t e l a a s g e n o e m d e stelling d e u r d a t d a a r a a n g e t o o n is dat h e r h a a l d e l i k e o t i t i s m e d i a w e l 'n i n v l o e d h e t o p die s e n t r a l e o u d i t i e w e v e r m o e n s v a n p r o e f p e r s o n e m e t s e n s o r i e s e i n t e g r a s i e p r o b l e m e e n w e l i n t e r m e v a n o n v o l d o e n d e b r e i n s t a m f u n k s i o n e r i n g . g e v o l g t r e k k i n g s e n a a n b e v e l i n g s a l h o e w e l die s t e e k p r o e f v a n h i e r d i e s t u d i e b e p e r k w a s , d u i die r e s u l t a t e d a a r o p d a t c h r o n i e s e otitis m e d i a w e l ' n i n v l o e d h e t op die s e n t r a l e o u d i t i e w e v e r m o e n s v a n k i n d e r s m e t sensoriese i n t e g r a s i e p r o b l e m e . d i e r e s u l t a t e w a t m e t die s e n t r a l e t o e t s b a t t e r y b y die p r o e f p e r s o n e m e t 'n g e s k i e d e n i s v a n otitis m e d i a v e r k r y is, d u i o p a f w y k e n d e r e s u l t a t e m e t b e t r e k k i n g tot die s e n t r a l e o u d i t i e w e v e r m o e n s , e n w e l i n t e r m e v a n onvold o e n d e b r e i n s t a m f u n k s i o n e r i n g . in die geval v a n die proefpers o n e s o n d e r g e s k i e d e n i s v a n otitis m e d i a , is n o r m a l e r e s u l t a t e i n al die t o e t s e v e r k r y , b e h a l w e i n geval v a n die ssi-toetsbatt e r y w a a r g e r i n g e a f w y k e n d e r e s u l t a t e d e u r die p r o e f p e r s o n e v e r t o o n is. l a a s g e n o e m d e r e s u l t a t e k a n egter v e r k l a a r w o r d i n die lig v a n die v i s u e e l o u d i t i e w e i n t e g r a s i e p r o b l e m e w a t k i n d e r s m e t s e n s o r i e s e i n t e g r a s i e p r o b l e m e o n d e r v i n d . i n t e r m e v a n i n t r a g r o e p k o r r e l a s i e s h e t d i e s e n t r a l e t o e t s b a t t e r y se res u l t a t e n i e g e d u i o p 'n b e d u i d e n d e verskil t u s s e n p r e s t a s i e v a n m a n l i k e e n v r o u l i k e p r o e f p e r s o n e in die t o e t s b a t t e r y n i e . ' n g r o t e r s t e e k p r o e f k a n m o o n t l i k geslagsverskille a a n die lig b r i n g . v a n u i t 'n k l i n i e s e o o g p u n t b l y k die r e s u l t a t e v a n h i e r d i e s t u d i e v e r a l v a n b e l a n g te w e e s . d a a r w o r d n i e n e t i n l i g t i n g o o r die i n v l o e d v a n otitis m e d i a o p die s e n t r a l e o u d i t i e w e v e r m o e n s v a n k i n d e r s m e t s e n s o r i e s e i n t e g r a s i e p r o b l e m e b e s k i k b a a r gestel n i e , m a a r o o k i n l i g t i n g o o r die rol v a n die o u d i o l o o g b y die k i n d m e t s e n s o r i e s e i n t e g r a s i e p r o b l e m e . i n s l u i t i n g v a n die o u d i o l o o g i n die m u l t i d i s s i p l i n e r e s p a n w a t die k i n d m e t sens o r i e s e i n t e g r a s i e p r o b l e m e h a n t e e r , k a n d e u r v r o e e i d e n tifikasie e n suksesvolle i n t e r v e n s i e tot die v o o r k o m i n g v a n die n a d e l i g e i n v l o e d v a n otitis m e d i a lei. v e r w y s i n g s ayres, a.j. sensory integration and the child. los angeles: western psychological services, 1973. ayres, a.j. sensory integration and learning disorders. los angeles: western psychological services, 1983. blager, f.b. "the effect of otitis media on speech and language development". seminars in speech, language and hearing, 3, 313320, 1982. gottlieb, m.e., zinkus, p.w. & thompson, m.a. "chronic middle-ear disease and auditory perceptual deficits". clinical pediatrics, 18(12), 725-732, 1979. hall, j.w. the acoustic reflex in central auditory dysfunction. in pinheiro, m.l. & musiek, f.e. (ed.): assessment of central auditory dysfunction foundations and clinical correlates. baltimore: williams and wilkens, 1985. hornsby, b. overcoming dyslexia. kenwyn. juta and co., ltd., 1984. jerger, j.f. "observations on auditory behaviour in lesion of central auditory pathways." archives of otolaryngology, 77, 797-806, 1960. jerger, j.f. & jerger, s. "auditory findings in brain-stem disorders." archives of otolarynyology, 99, 342l349, 1974. jerger, j.f. & jerger s. auditory disorders a manual for clinical evaluation. boston. little brown inc., 1981. jerger, s. evaluation of central auditory function in children. in keith, r.w. (ed.): central auditory and language disorders in children. houston: college hill press, 1981. johnson, d.j. considerations in the assessment of central auditory disorders in learning disabled children. in keith, r.w. (ed.): central auditory and language disorders in children. houston: college hill press, 1981. ' katz,j. & wilde, l. auditory perceptual disorders in children. inkatz, . j. (ed.): clinical handbook of audioloyy. baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. mcshane, d. & mitchell, j. "middle ear disease, hearing loss and educational problems of american indian children." journal of american indian education, 7-11, 1979. musiek, e.m. & pinheiro, m.l. dichotic speech tests in the detection of central auditory dysfunction. in pinheiro, m.l. & musiek, f.e. (eds.): assessment of central auditory dysfunction foundations and clinical correlates. baltimore; williams & wilkens, 1975. noffsinger, d., martinez, c.d. & schaefer, a.b. puretone techniques in evaluation of central auditory function. in katz, j. (ed.): handbook of clinical audiology. baltimore, williams & wilkens, 1985. northern, j.l. & downs, m.p. hearing in children. baltimore: williams & wilkens, 1984. northern, j.l. gabbard, s.a. & kinder, d.l. the acoustic reflex. in katz, j. (ed.): handbook of clinical audiology. baltimore: williams & wilkens, 1985. owrid, h.l. "hearing impairment and verbal attainment in primary school children." education research, 12, 209-214, 1970. paradise, j.l. "long term effects of short term hearing loss menace or myth?" pediatrics, 71, 647-648, 1983. | rintelman, w.f. monaural speech tests in the detection of central 1 auditory disorders. in pinheiro, m.l. & musiek, f.e. (ed.):! assessment of central dysfunction foundations and clinical cor i relates. baltimore: williams & wilkens, 1985. j rubin, r.j. "the effects of recurrent middle ear effusion in pre-school | years on language and learning." audiology in practice, 1 (supι plement 3), 5-7, 1984. i sak, r.j. & ruben, r.j. "effects of recurrent middle ear effusion in preschool years on language and learing ."journal of developmental i and behavioural pediatrics, 3, 7-77, 1982. ί stephens, s.d.g. & thornton, a.r.d. "subjective and electrophysiologic tests in brainstem lesions". archives of otolarynyology, 102, 608-612, 1976. webster, d.b. effects of peripheral hearing losses on the audiotry brainstem. in lasky, e.z. &katz, j. (ed.): central auditory processing disorders of speech, language and learning. baltimore:' university park press, 1983. willeford, j. a. sentence tests of central auditory dysfunction. in katz, j. (ed.): handbook of clinical audioloyy. baltimore: williams & wilkens, 1985. / zinkus, p.w. "psychoeducational and sequelae of chronic otitis media." seminars in speech, language and heariny, 3, 305312, 1982. ' / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 segmental speech timing at the phoneme and syllable levels in english and afrikaans speaking white south african children carol l oosthuizen, ph d (cape town) department of otorhinolaryngology university of stellenbosch abstract two aspects of segmental timing were acoustically measured in the speech production of30 english and 30 afrikaans speaking white south african children. these were votand medial stop closure duration in vcv nonsense syllables. in addition, medial vowel duration in cvc nonsense syllables was measured in english speakers. comparisons were made between younger (mean age 4.25 years) andolder (meanage 6.5 years) subjects in each language group. graphical statistical methods revealed certain trends in the data. english speakers employed shortand long-lag votfor voiced and voiceless stops while afrikaans speakers used two short-lag categories. contextually determined right-to-left timing effects were identified which were in agreement with the literature, with different rates of acquisition of medial stop closure duration rules being observed between the language groups. opsomming twee aspekte van segmentele tydreeling in die spraakproduksie van 30engels en 30 afrikaanssprekende blanke suid-afrikaansekinders is akoesties gemeet. dit was stemaanvangstyd en tydsduur van mediale plosiewe sluiting in vkv onsinlettergrepe. tydsduur van mediale vokale in'kvk onsinlettergrepe is ook in engelssprekendes gemeet. vergelykings is ook gemaak tussenjonger (gemiddelde ouderdom 4.25 jaar) en ouer (gemiddelde ouderdom 6.5 jaar) proefpersone in elke taalgroep. dit is bevind dat engelssprekendes gebruik maak van kort en lang vertraging in stemaanvangstyd vir stemhebbende en stemlose plosiewe, terwyl afrikaanssprekendes twee kort vertragingskategoriee gebruik. regs-tot-links konteksgebonde neigings wat in segmentele tydreeling geidentifiseer is, het met die literatuur ooreengestem terwyl verskillende tempo's van verwerwing van reels vir tydsduur van mediale,plosiewe tussen die taalgroepe waargeneem is. temporal aspects of speech production have been highlighted by kent (1976) as possibly "j., the most critical factor in skilled motor performance." (ρ 43δ[) the development of speech timxiflg is thought to reflect neuromaturation of the speech mechanism and has important implications for many branches of the study of communication disorders. kent points out that phonetic judgments are only gross indicators of motor speech devejppment and that more sensitive parameters of motor skills, such as the acquisition of segmental timing rules, continue to develop after children have acquired the basic phonological system. some developmental patterns in segmental timing have been demonstrated (inter alia, by kewley-port & preston, 1974; disimoni, 1974 a,b,c; tingley & allen, 1975;menyuk&klatt, 1975; gilbert, 1976; gilbert & johnson, 1978; smith, 1978; macken & barton, 1979; flege, mccutcheon & smith, 1987) but the still inadequate body of research on the subject precludes a comprehensive description._ development in the various levels of speech timing has not been fully investigated. cooper (1977) identified three levels on which children learn control of speech timing: 1. the phonetic-phonemic level, such as voice onset time (vot), i.e the time taken from the release of a stop plosive closure to the onset of voicing for the post-consonantal vowel. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 2. the syllable level, comprising rules for the timing of sounds within syllables. this can be illustrated by a temporal compensation rule which states that a longerthan-average vowel must be followed by a shorter-thanaverage consonant. 3. the sentence level. both children and adults seem to preplan the timing of segments within utterances, e.g. the longer the utterances, the shorter the individual segments. a fourth level of speech timing may be added (dalton & hardcastle, 1977), viz.: 4. the prosodic level, i.e. transition smoothness between intonation units. it is not yet known whether measures of timing variables in very young children would predict later motor speech performance. the intriguing question of whether or not there is a relationship between speech timing development and speech disorders has only been addressed in a preliminary way with regard to articulation disorders (weismer & elbert, 1982), phonological disorders (catts & jensen, 1983; smit & bernthall, 1983), cleft palate (forner, 1983), hearing impairment (stark, 1972), delayed language development (bord & wilson, 1980) and stuttering (inter alia, disimoni, 1974c; ® sasha 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 carol l oosthuizen starkweather et al. 1976; starkweather & myers, 1979; oosthuizen, 1986; adams, 1987; mcknight & cullinan, 1987). a description of normal development should be the precursor to the study of timing problems in speech disorders. no normative study of speech timing in south african children has yet been published and this report represents a limited attempt to explore a vast subject. most white south african children speak english or afrikaans, which have been the two official languages since 1925. owing to historical and geographical influences, south african english is a dialect which differs from forms of english found in other countries (lanham, 1967). afrikaans is a germanic language which was derived from dutch. it cannot be assumed that the findings of studies of speech timing using speakers of other english dialects or germanic languages can automatically be applied to speakers of south african english or afrikaans. separate investigations are merited which might eventually yield valuable insights into the relationships between the development of speech timing, dialect and language. the three aspects of expressive speech timing which form the topic of this report fall within the phonetic-phonemic and syllable levels. they are syllable-initial vot, syllable-medial stop closure duration and syllable-medial vowel duration. these segmental timing elements were studied in the speech of english and afrikaans speaking white south african children and formed part of a larger study directed toward the speech of stutterers (oosthuizen, 1986). they will be discussed as three separate experimental procedures carried out on the same subjects, experiment three using english speaking subjects only. experiment one. aim the aim of experiment one was to identify normal trends for vot production of syllable-initial stop plosives in the experimental groups. vot is an important dimension of segmental timing, since it i s a valid measure to differentiate phonemic voicing categories in many languages. most studies of vot production in children have employed very small subject samples, nevertheless some developmental trends have emerged. kewly-port and preston (1974), studying american english children (n3), found that, initially, short-lag vots predominated with a gradual increase in longlag vots. those findings were confirmed by macken and barton (1979) who noted that their 4 subjects had reached adult contrast values by 2.4 years. however, a small sample of canadian english children failed to demonstrate adult / t / values by three years (gilbert 1976). in reviewing the literature, kent (1976) concluded that vot attains adult stability around eight years of age. ί. method subjects 30 english and 30 afrikaans speaking white south african children from monolingual homesin the cape peninsula were divided into younger and older age groups (see table 1). the subjects had no known medical or neurological disorders, nor any history of communication problems and had passed hearing screening tests. table 1: subject groupings w i t h mean ages in years and months subgroup (n-15) mean age age range 1. older english speakers 6,10 yrs 6,5 to 7,6 2. older afrikaans speakers 6,9 yrs 6,4 to 7,10 3. younger english speakers 4,4 yrs 4,0 to 5,0 4. younger afrikaans speakers 4,1 yrs 3,1 to 4,8 procedures for data collection four productions of each of the following cvc nonsense syllables were elicited from each subject and recorded on audio tape: /paf/ /baf/ /taf/ /daf/ the subjects "were tested individually in a quiet environment. the syllables were used in a story context by an adult native speaker of each language, following a procedure described by hawkins (1973), after which deferred imitations were elicited from the children. data analysis storage oscilloscopic displays of the first three clear, isolated productions of each of the syllables for each subject were obtained using an electronic memory (biomation model 1015) and tektronix 510 3n oscilloscope. judgment of speech segmental boundaries in the display followed the guidelines laid down by shoup and pfeifer (1976) and segments were measured in milliseconds. the vot of each production was classified as falling into the short-, long-lag, or voicing lead categories. in their seminal study of adult native speakers of eleven languages, lisker and abramson (1964) identified three categories of vot values: prevoicing (voicing lead): -125 to zero msecs ι short-lag: zero to /25 msecs ι long-lag: /60 to /100 j it is n o w generally accepted that a vot boundary of approx[ imately /35 msecs separates the shortand long-lag categories in english (cooper, 1977), a figure which corresponds witli the perceptual boundary. j statistical treatment 1 owing to the small number of subjects (15) in each subgroup, and given the exploratory nature of the study, graphical statistical methods were applied. multiple box plots were used to display the central tendency and spread of the mean, the range, maximum and minimum scores for each production of each syllable. results and discussion ^ figure 1 shows summary box plots illustrating the relative vot values for the /paf/-/baf/ cognate pair in the four experimental groups. comment on the distribution refers to the interquartile distance, chosen as a measure of spread, and the median, chosen as a central value. these parameters were / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) segmental speech timing at the phoneme and syllable levels 5 regarded as preferable to the standard deviation or arithmetic mean, since many of the distributions were asymmetrical; they have the additional advantage of being little affected by outlying scores. outlying scores are represented on the distribution by asterisks in a vertical column. results for the /taf/-/daf/ cognate pair (not shown) were comparable and are included in the comments. ker (1974). this relative reduction of vot for voiceless syllable initial stop plosives reflects the local dialect, since the older english speakers' scores were comparable to adult data obtained in an unpublished pilot study. speculation regarding the various influences on the local dialect which could affect segmental timing would be premature as ideas in this regard are currently under review (lass, personal communication 1986). older older younger younger • 8 0 enorsh speakers afrikaans speakers english speakers alrikaans speakers i v o t i n m s e c s • 5 0 ' .40 +30 +20 +10 0 -10-20" 3 0 " 4 0 5 0 6 0 ψ figure 1 summary box plots showing vot values for the /paf/-/baf/ cognate pair in the four groups of subjects. a few extreme prevoicing scores have been omitted in older afrikaans speakers. while there was a degree of overlap between vots for voiced and voiceless cognate pairs, older english speakers made clearer distinctions between them than did their afrikaans peers. south african english appears to be a two category dialect employing shortand long-lag vot contrasts for voiced and voiceless stops respectively. afrikaans speakers appeared to use two sets of unaspirated stops for 'voiced' and voiceless' sounds all of which fell into the short-lag category, a finding which provides documentary evidence of a feature which is familiar to afrikaans phoneticians (de villiers, /1967). older english speakers sho lag vots on voiceless stops ved a greater trend toward longthan did younger ones. this is in agreement with the reported developmental findings, that children have to learn to lengthen vot. afrikaans speakers did not show vot lengthening for voiceless stops with age, since they did not employ the long-lag category. analysis of multiple box plots of the range (not shown) indicated increased variability of vot on voiceless stops with age in the case of english speakers. this was attributed to the inherent scope of the long-lag category, which permits a wide variety of acceptable scores. most afrikaans speakers showed decreased variability of production with age; which agrees with previous findings (eguchi and hirsch, 1969; disimoni, 1974a, 1974b; tingley and allen, 1975; zlatin and koenigsknecht, 1976). however, some individuals showed increased variability for voiced stops owing to the development of voicing lead as a stylistic variant. in the case of the south african english children, the means for long-lag vot were considerably smaller than those reported in the literature for american english children, yet they fell into the adult long-lag range identified by abramson and lisexperiment two disimoni (1974a, 1974b), studying american english children, found that the influence of phonetic context on segmental durations appeared between three and six years of age and approached, but did not yet reach adult levels, by nine years. in vcv syllables, the stop closure durations of both voiced and voiceless stops were found to be greater in / i / t h a n in / a / environments. the present experiment set out to determine whether similar trends characterized the speech of south african children. procedures for data collection for experiments 2 and 3. t w o randomized word lists, each containing eight nonsense syllables and yielding three nonconsecutive productions of each syllable by each subject were audio recorded. in experiment 2 these were vcv syllables: /apa/ /aba/ /ata/ /ada/ /ipi/ /ibi/ /iti/ /idi/ the task was one of imitation of an adult speaker, recorded in the case of older subjects and live voice presentation for younger subjects, the syllables being preceded by a carrier phrase. the preconsonantal vowels were stressed in experiment 2. data analysis t w o measures were taken from each production of each syllable, viz. whole syllable duration and the closure phase of the medial stop plosive. results and discussion figures 2 5 contain summary multiple box plots illustrating the central tendency and spread of stop closure duration scores for the experimental syllables in the four groups. nonsense syllables idi ibi ata slop closure 129 • duration in msecs 109 figure 2 summary box plots showing the central tendency and spread of stop closure durations i n vcv syllables in older english speakers. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 carol l oosthuizen stop 159 closure duration in msecs 124 η 532nonsense syllables u bl ill figure 3 summary box plots showing the central tendency and spread of stop closure durations in the vcv syllables in older afrikaans speakers. 223slop closure 157cbratlon in maeca 12490.35 7 2 24:0 • aba id bl ata apa kl ty 1 τ τ figure 4 summary box plots showing the central tendency and spread of stop closure durations in vcv nonsense syllables in younger english speakers. slop 201 closure duration in msecs 163 η 48.0 • nonsense syllables idi ibi ata apa figure 5 summary box plots showing the central tendency and spread of stop closure durations in vcv syllables in younger afrikaans speakers. sistent with the results of acoustic studies (schwartz, 1969; disimoni, 1974a; suen and beddoes, 1974; smith, 1978) as well as a physiological study by butcher and weiher (1976). schwartz (1969) attributed the shorter stop closure duration prior to / a / to coarticulation, the earlier consonant release compensating for the greater distance to be covered in order to reach the low vowel position compared with the high one. in contrast to canadian english adults (suen & beddoes, 1974) the south african children showed greater mean'stop closure duration differences between cognate pairs for alveolar sounds than for bilabials. perhaps this place-of-articulation effect results from relatively poor motor capabilities of the tongue tip in children. the younger south african english speakers had a mean closure duration difference between voiced and voiceless stops of 16,7 msecs, which is far below the values reported by smith (1978) for american english children of comparable ages. these discrepancies probably reflect dialectal differences, although methodological factors cannot be discounted. there appeared to be a developmental trend in the direction of lengthening of medial stop closure duration of voiceless stops. this voicing distinction seemed to be acquired earlier in both vowel environments in afrikaans speakers compared with english speakers. a second age-related trend, which was also interpreted as reflecting a developmental pattern, was toward lengthening of medial stop closure duration in the high vowel environment. variability of timing production was greater amongst younger speakers compared with older ones, confirming the reported trend in the literature. whole syllable durations did not decrease with age, however. another unexpected finding was that of greater whole syllable durations in english than in afrikaans speakers. this cannot simply be attributed to longer vots in the english group. vot lengthens with age, yet whole syllable durations were longer in younger than in older speakers. since stop closure durations per se, which included transitions, did not differ between the language groups, it seems that the english speakers may have been using longer vowels than did their afrikaans counterparts. the final vowel is generally thought to exert most influence on medial stop closure duration (anderson, 1975), but there is conflicting evidence regarding the influence of the preceding vowel (schwartz, 1969; butcher & weiher, 1976). since both vowels were the same in each syllable, it is not possible to determine which was affecting medial stop closure durations. j the lack of consistent age related trends for whole syllable duration is in agreement with kubaska and keating (1981) who observed that, provided that the positions of words in utterances were controlled, and once children had reached the two-word utterance stage of development, word durations did not change much over time in american english children (n3). experiment three no differences emerged between the older language groups with respect to stop closure duration trends. both groups showed longer medial stop closure durations for voiceless compared with voiced stops. stop closure durations were also longer in / i / than in /a/environments. these findings are conthis experiment was performed on english speakers only, since final voiced stops and fricatives do not normally occur in afrikaans (combrink & de stadlef, 1987). vowels which precede voiced stops have been, shown to be approximately 100 msecs longer than those preceding final the south african journal of communication , disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) segmental speech timing at the phoneme and syllable levels 7 voiceless stops in adult speech (house & fairbanks, 1953; peterson & lehiste, 1960). naeser (1970), smith (1978) and krause (1982) have demonstrated similar trends in 2-3 year old american english children. the phenomenon has also been identified in older children by disimoni (1974b) who indicated that development was in the direction of vowel lengthening for voiced consonants rather than shortening for voiceless consonants. this experiment was aimed at determining the trends characterizing the english speaking subgroups in terms of medial vowel duration relative to the consonant environment in cvc nonsense syllables. data collection and analysis the word lists contained these cvc nonsense syllables: /pip / / p a p / /sis/ /sas/ /bib / /bab/ /ziz/ /zaz/ measures were taken of vowel and whole syllable durations. results and discussion figures 6 and 7 show summary box plots indicating vowel durations for the eight syllables in the two subgroups. results showed that vowels were longer in sibilant than in stop plosive contexts as well as in voiced compared with voiceless consonant environments. 231 vowel 191 duration in msecs nonsense syllables zaz ziz pip k a 30.0figure β summary box plots showing the central tendency and spread of vowel durations in cvc syllables in older english speakers. contrary to other reports (reviewed by kent, 1976), segment durations did not decrease with age. however, an age-related trend was identified in the increasing differential vowel duration relative to both voicing and manner-of-articulation of the voiced consonant context. decreasing variability of performance with age was found on measures of whole syllable duration. general discussion the segmental phonetic level of speech timing, which would include vot, is considered by dalton and hardcastle (1977) to be dependent on aerodynamic coordination. the relatively late development of long-lag vot in speakers of those languages which use it might be attributed to its greater neurovowel 214duration in msecs nonsense syllables zaz ziz pip pap bib bab τ figure 7 summary box plots showing the central tendency and spread of v o w e l durations in cvc syllables in younger english speakers. physiological complexity, compared with short-lag vot (cooper, 1977). however, lisker (1975) suggested that vot was influenced by an interaction between biological restraints and phonological rules. the general decrease in mean segment duration which has been reported as a function of age (smith, 1978; kent & forner, 1980) and the development oflong-lag vot might largely reflect increasing neuromuscular capabilities. it has been proposed (suen & beddoes, 1974) that the relatively late development of longer stop closure durations for voiceless stops reflects the increased muscular effort required to produce them. this argument is challenged by the cross-language findings in the south african children, the relatively early development of longer closure durations in afrikaans speakers suggesting that phonological considerations may be important. it seems that certain elements of speech timing are singled out for this kind of development in the age group studied, probably on the basis of their phonological significance. this issue of the relative contribution of biomechanical and phonologically-programmed influences on segmental speech timing is further complicated by the probable lack of a simple one-to-one relationship between the underlying phonological segmental length specification and the observable phonetic duration. allen (1973) has suggested that sources of variation would include speech tempo, biological restraints (such as j a w mass) and aerodynamic factors. conclusion in the main, the segmental timing trends identified in the south african subjects agreed with those reported in the literature, i.e.: 1. increasing vot for voiceless stops as a function of age in english speakers. 2. longer medial stop closure durations in vcv syllables when the stop was voiceless and in high vowel environments. a developmental trend toward the acquisition of differential stop closure durations were observed. 3. longer medial vowel durations in cvc syllables in voiced and voiceless sibilant consonant environments and a developmental trend in this regard in english speakdie suid-aftikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. so, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 carol l oosthuizen south african english was identified as a two-category language employing shortand long-lag vots for voiced and voiceless stops respectively. afrikaans speakers appeared to use two short-lag categories with random voicing lead on voiced stops in some subjects. the fact that there were mixed findings regarding decreasing variability of production as a function of age may be attributed to the relatively small age gap between the older and younger groups of subjects. differences between speakers of the two languages with respect to rate and, to a lesser extent, degree of acquisition of anticipatory contextual timing effects highlighted the importance of comparing cross-language data when interpreting research results in this area of study. the research findings on segmental timing effects in the connected speech of adult speakers (chrystal & house, 1988) suggest that due caution should be observed when attempting to generalize from data derived from isolated words or syllables. such restricted stimuli may bear a close relation only to utterancerinitial and prepausal words in connected speech, but this remains to be determined. references abramson, a.s. and lisker, l. voice-timing perception in spanish word-initial stops ./. phonetics, 5, 1-7, 1974. adams, m.r. voice onsets and segment durations of normal speakers and beginning stutterers. j. fluency dis., 12, 133-139, 1987. allen, g.d. segmental timing control in speech production./. phonetics, 1, 219-237, 1973. anderson, b.w. ballistic control of rhythmic articulatory movements in normal speech. n.y. acad, of sciences, 263, 236-243, 1975. bord, z. and wilson, h. acquisition of the voicing contrast by language-delayed and normal-speaking children. j. speech hear. res., 23, 152-161, 1980. butcher, a. and weiher, e. an electropalatographic investigation of coarticulation in vcv sequences./. phonetics, 4, 59-74, 1976. catts, h.w. and jensen, p.j. speech timing of phonologically disordered children: voicing contrast of initial and final stop consonants. j. speech hear. res. 26, 501-510, 1983. combrinkj.g.h. and de stadler, l.g. afrikaanse fonologie. johannesburg: macmillan, 1987. cooper, w.e. the development of speech timing. in s.j. segalowitz and f.a. gruber (eds.) language development and neurological theory. new york : academic, 1977. crystal, t.h. and house, a.s. segmental durations in connectedspeech signals: current results. /. acoust. soc. am., 83, 15531573, 1988. dalton, p. and hardcastle, w.j. disorders of fluency. london: arnold, 1977. de villiers, m. afrikaanse klankleer. cape town : balkema, 1967. disimoni, f.g. influence of vowel environment on the duration of consonants in the speech of three-, six-, and nine-year old children./. acoust. soc. am., 55, 360-361, 1974a. disimoni, f.g. influence of consonant environment on duration of vowels in the speech of three-, six-, and nine-year old children./ acoust. soc. am, 55, 362-363, 1974b. disimoni, f.g. preliminary study of certain timing relations in the speech of stutterers. j. acoust. soc. am., 56, 695-696, 1974c. disimoni, f.g. some preliminary observations on temporal compensation in the speech of children./. acoust. soc. am., 56, 697699, 1974d. eguchi, s. and hirsh, i.j. development of speech sounds in children. ·' acta oto-laryngologica, suppl. 257, 1969. flege j.e., mccutcheon, m.j. and smith, s.c. the development of skill in producing word-final english stops./. acoust. soc. am., 82, 433-447, 1987. forner, l.l. speech segment durations produced by five and six year old speakers with and without cleft palate. cleft palate j., 20, 185-198, 1983. gilbert, j.h.v. a voice onset time analysis of apical stop production in three-year-olds./. child lang., 4, 103-110, 1976. gilbert, j.h.v. and johnson, c.e. temporal and sequential constraints on six-year-old's phonological productions: some observations on the 'ambliance' phenomenon./. child lang., 5, 101-112, 1978. hawkins, s. temporal coordination of consonants in the speech of children : preliminary data./. phonetics, 1, 181-217, 1973. house, a.s. and fairbanks, g. the influence of consonant environment upon the secondary acoustical characteristics of vowels./. acoust. soc. am., 25, 105-113, 1953. kent, r.d. anatomical and neuromuscular maturation of the speech mechanism: evidence from acoustic studies/. speech hear. res., 19, 421-447, 1976. kent, r.d. and forner, l.l. speech segment durations in sentence recitations by children and adults./. phonetics, 8, 157-168, 1980. kewly-port, d. and preston, m.s. early apical stop production: a voice onset time analysis. /. phonetics, 2, 195 -210, 1974. krause, s.e. developmental use of vowel duration as a cue to postvocalic consonant voicing./. speech hear. res. 25, 388-398, 1982. kubaska, c. a. and keating, p.a. word duration in early child speech./. speech hear. res., 24, 615-621, 1981. lanham, l.w. the pronunciation of south african english. cape town: balkema, 1967. lass, r. personal communication department of linguistics, university of cape town, december 1986. lisker, l. is it vot or a first-formant transition detector?/. acoust. soc. am., 57, 1547-1551, 1975. lisker, l. and abrahamson, a.s. a cross-language study of voicing in initial stops : acoustical measurements. word, 20, 384-422, 1964. mack, m. voicing-dependent vowel duration in english and french : monolingual and bilingual production/. acoust. soc. am., 71, 173-178, 1982. macken, m.a. and barton, d. the acquisition of the voicing contrast in english : a study of voice onset time in word-initial stop consonants./. child lang., 7, 41-74, 1979. mcknight, r. and cullinan, w. subgroups of stuttering children: speech and voice reaction times, segmental durations, and naming latencies./. fluency dis., 12, 217-233, 1987. menyuk, p. and klatt, m. voice onset time in consonant cluster production by children and adults. /. child lang., 2, 223-231, 1975. naeser, m.a. the influence of initial and final consonants on vowel duration in cvc syllables. technical report no. 130, wisconsin research and development center for cognitive learning, university of wisconsin, 1970. oosthuizen, c.l. a cross-linguistic study of certain temporal features of speech in stuttering and nonstuttering children. unpublished doctoral thesis, university of cape town, 1986. peterson, g.e. and lehiste, i. duration of syllable nuclei in english./. acoust. soc. am., 32, 693-703, 1960. schwartz, m. influence of vowel environment on the duration of /s/ and if i. j. acoust. soc. am., 46, 480-483, 1969. i shoup, j.e. and pfeifer, l.l. acoustic characteristics of speech sounds. in n.j. lass (ed.) contemporary issues in experimental phonetics. new york : academic, 1976. smit, a.b. and bernthall, j.e. voicing contrasts and their phonological implications in the speech of articulation-disordered children1. /. speech hear. res, 26, 486-500, 1983. i smith, b.l. temporal aspects of english speech production: a developmental perspective./. phonetics, 6, 37-68, 1978. | stark, r. some features of the vocalizations of young deaf children. in| j.f. bosma (ed.) third symposium on oral sensation and perception: the mouth of the infant springfield, 111.: thomas, 1972. starkweather, c.w. and myers, m. duration of subsegments within the intervocalic interval in stutterers and nonstutterers. / . fluency dis., 4, 205-214, 1979. starkweather, c. w., hirschman, p. and tannenbaum, r.s. latency of vocalization onset: stutterers versus nonstutterers. /. speech hear. res., 19, 481-492, 1976. suen, c.y. and beddoes, m.p. the silent interval of stop consonants. lang. speech, 17, 126-134, 1974. tingley, b.m. and allen, g.d. development of speech timing'control in children. child dev., 46, 186-194, 1975. ' weismer, g. and elbert, m. temporal characteristics of the "functionally" misarticulated /s/in 4 6-year-old children./. speech hear. res., 25, 275-287, 1982. zimmermann, g.n. and sapon, s.m. note on vowel duration seen cross-linguistically. j. acoust. soc. am., 30, 152-153, 1958. zlatin, m.a. and koenigsknecht, r.a. development of the voicing contrast : a comparison of voice onset time in stop perception and production./. speech hear. res., 19, 93-111, 1976. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 57 use of the cid w22 as a south african english speech discrimination test wayne j. wilson and selvarani moodley department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract south africa currently lacks a pre-recorded south african english (sae) specific speech discrimination test. in the absence of such a test, the sae speaker recording (tygerberg recording) of the american (usa) english (ae) cid w22 wordlists in combination with the original american cid w22 normative data is the most widely used alternative. the reliability and validity of this method, however, has never been formally assessed. this study assessed the performance of 15 normal hearing, female, first language sae speakers on the first two full-lists of tygerberg cid w22 recording at 20, 30, 40, 50, 60 and 70 dbspl, and compared their scores to the american cid w22 wordlist normative data. overall, the south african subjects performed worse than the original american normative data at the lower presentation intensities (<50 dbspl). use of the tygerberg cid w22 recording with the original american cid w22 normative data for near threshold assessment of sae speaking subjects was therefore concluded to be problematic. use at suprathreshold intensities (> 40 dbspl), however, was considered a viable option. these results reiterate the need for large scale, south african specific normative studies for the cid w22 wordlists if they are to continue their role as the dominant speech discrimination wordlists in south africa. key words: special discrimination, cid w22 wordlists, american (usa) english, south african english. introduction the cid auditory test w22, from the united states of america, represents the most widely used, basic audiological speech discrimination test in south africa. the test's popularity appears to be due mainly to its widespread use in the usa only, however, as opposed to any formal research demonstrating the validity and reliability of its use in the south african context. these factors, support for and criticisms against the cid w22, and the general problems of applying a non-south african specific speech discrimination test to the southj african population have been discussed previously (wilson, jones & fridjhon, 1998). the cid w22 was originally designed as an improvement on the psycho-acoustic laboratories phonetically balanced 50 word lists (pal-pb 50) speech discrimination test. it improved phonetic balance (lehiste & peterson, 1955), equality of difficulty between lists (brewer & resnick, 1983) and degree of familiarity (brewer & resnick, 1983; hirsh et al., 1952). the test consists of 4 lists of 50 monosyllabic words with six randomizations of each list. the words were originally chosen on the basis of being representative of north american (usa) english, with all but one of the chosen words appearing on the thorndike "list of most frequently used words" (hirsh et al., 1952). the chosen words were also checked for phonetic balance according to studies of syllable-consonant-vowel distribution in american english (ae) (hirsh et al., 1952). whilst originally created as 50 word full-lists, users of the cid w22 wordlists recognised almost immediately the benefits of reducing test time by administering 25 word half-lists (martin & forbis, 1978; edgerton & danhauer, 1979). these shorter lists remain in widespread clinical use despite being prone to higher variability (and therefore poorer reliability) and exacerbation of the already difficult phonetic balance problem (ostergard, 1983). the original cid w22 standardisation information was obtained on 15 normal hearing, ae speakers. all four lists were randomly presented at 10 db intervals from 20 dbspl to 70 dbspl. the resultant group "performance-intensity" plot is shown in hirsh et al. (1952) and represents the averaged performance of the subjects on all four full-lists at each presentation intensity. the actual scores obtained are not given and are extrapolated from the hirsh et al. (1952) plot. the success of the cid w22 wordlists in the usa lead to their introduction and widespread use in south africa. in an attempt to make these standardised cid w22 wordlists more suitable for the south african english (sae) speaking population, a sae speaker recording of the original wordlists was made at tygerberg hospital in the western cape. technical information on the tygerberg cid w22 recording is not available. what is known is that the recording is comprised of the 4 original american wordlists of 50 words each, all read by a south african adult male who is a first language speaker of sae. each word is preceded by the carrier phrase "say the word", the inter-stimulus interval die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 wayne j. wilson and selvarani moodley is 4 seconds and serves as the subject's response period. it must be noted at this stage that a popular alternative to the tygerberg cid w22 recording is to present the wordlists via monitored live voice (mlv). whilst the mlv presentation may provide a partial solution to speakerlistener accent mismatch, it has been widely criticised for its poor test-retest reliability (carhart, 1965; brandy, 1966; kreul, bell & nixon, 1969 and northern & hattler, 1974) and the inherent problem of comparing results using monitored live voice to standardisation information obtained using recorded versions of the test (mendel & danhauer, 1997). the recorded version of a test keeps the tester voice constant therefore improving test-retest reliability and allows valid comparisons to standardised normative data. because of this, testing using the tygerberg recording was preferred in this study. south african normative data for the tygerberg recording of the cid w22 has not been published. as a result, the performance of sae speaking subjects on the tygerberg recording, whether it be half-list of full-list performance, is typically compared to the original ae subject, averaged full-list normative data for the original american cid w22 recording. such comparisons add obvious reliability and validity problems to the already stretched reliability and validity of using an american designed and standardised test on south african subjects. formal assessment of the reliability and validity of using the tygerberg cid w22 wordlist recording with the original american cid w22 normative data to assess first language sae speakers is needed if this protocol is to remain the dominant speech discrimination test protocol in south africa. methodology aims this study aimed to: 1. measure the performance of 15 female, first language south african english (sae) speakers on the first two full lists, and the resultant first four half-lists, of the sae recorded version (tygerberg recording) of the cid auditory test w22 at stimulus intensities of 20, 30, 40, 50, 60 and 70dbspl. 2. compare the sae speaker results to the original american cid w22 wordlist normative data. 3. compare the performance of the south african subjects only, between full-lists and between half-lists at each presentation intensity, and within full-lists and half-lists between each adjacent presentation intensity. 4. comment on the suitability of using the tygerberg recording of the cid w22 wordlists, with comparison with the original american cid w22 normative data, to assess first language sae speakers. subjects subject selection criteria subjects were selected using a convenience sampling technique. for acceptance into the study, subjects were required to conform to the following criteria (as confirmed by audiometric testing and self-report): (i) aged 17 40 years. this controlled for the well-recognised effects of paediatric and geriatric age ranges on performance on speech discrimination tasks (hall, 1983). (ii) female gender. this criterion was included for ease of subject selection, as it was deemed unlikely that a sample balanced for gender could easily be obtained, (iii) english as first-language, (iv) resident in gauteng, south africa as according to fuller (1987), subjects for research in the area of speech audiometry should be native to the local area, (v) hearing thresholds < 5dbhl at 250, 500,1000, 2000,4000 and 8000 hz in the test ear. (v) no significant history, past or present, of: hearing impairment; speech or language impairment; tinnitus; ear infections; noise exposure; or; family history of hearing problems, (vi) no previous knowledge of, or experience regarding the cid w22 wordlists. subject description an initial sample of 18 female, first language sae speaking subjects, all resident in the gauteng area, was obtained. three subjects were excluded on the basis of pure tone threshold criteria. the final 15 subjects ranged in age from 18.6 to 31.4 years, with a mean age of 23.9 ± 4.4 years. educational levels included two subjects with secondary school education and 13 subjects with or receiving tertiary level education. the better ear only was tested in each subject with a final 8 right and 7 left ears tested. test environment, equipment and recorded material testing was conducted in a two-room soundproof booth at the university of the witwatersrand speech and hearing clinic in gauteng. a one-way mirror enabled observation of subjects during testing. the tygerberg cid w22 wordlists were presented via an aiwa audiocassette player coupled with a grason stadler gsi16 audiometer. presentation was through tdh39 headphones with mxar41 cushions. measurement procedures and data collection subjects first filled in a case history questionnaire. pure tone air conduction thresholds for 250-8000 hz were then obtained for both ears using a standard hughson-westlake test procedure. if all selection criteria were met, the subject's better ear was chosen for speech testing. speech discrimination testing was conducted using the first two of the four tygerberg cid w22 pre-recorded full wordlists at 20,30,40,50,60 and 70 dbspl. administration; of all four full-lists at each intensity was not conducted as1 it was thought that this would lead to unacceptably high, levels of test duration and subject fatigue. all testing was. conducted by a final year ba (speech and hearing therapy) student who has had clinical training in audiometric procedures, under the supervision of an audiologist registered with health professions council of south africa. the speech discrimination testing followed a similar protocol to that used by the central institute for the deaf to normalize the original cid w22 wordlists (hirsh et al., 1952). the two full-lists were split and presented as four half-lists at each of the six presentation intensities in a pseudo-randomised order to prevent a particular list from occurring several times in succession. the same set of instructions was given to all subjects through the headphones: "you are going to hear sentences. i want you to repeat the last word of each sentence." the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) use of the cid w22 as a south african english speech discrimination test 59 subjects' responses were recorded and scored on-line by the tester. an all-or-none scoring procedure was used where the subject had to perceive the entire word correctly in order to receive credit. a correct response obtained a score of 2% for the full lists and 4% for the half-lists. subjects were allowed to rest at their request at any time during the test procedure in order to reduce fatigue (mendel & danhauer, 1997). data analysis south african english speakers' scores for the two full and four half-list tygerberg cid w22 wordlists were visually compared to the original american, averaged scores on the american cid w22 full-lists [as extrapolated from the graphs provided in hirsh et al. (1952)]. descriptive differences were identified. the south african subjects' scores on the tygerberg cid w22 wordlists were then analysed for differences between: mean scores on the two full-lists, for each presentation intensity separately, using two-tailed t-tests for dependent samples at the 5% significance level. mean scores on the four half-lists, for each presentation intensity separately, using one-way repeated measure anova and tukeys honest significant difference analyses at the 5% significance level. mean scores between each adjacent pair of presentation intensities (20-30,30-40,40-50,50-60 and 60-70 dbspl), for each full-list and half-list separately, using repeated wilcoxon matched pairs analyses at the 1% significance level. score variances between presentation intensities for each full and half-list separately, and between full and halff u l l l i s t 1 f u l l l i s t 2 30 40 50 d b s p l half-list 1 1 half-list 1 2 20 30 40 50 60 70 d b s p l 20 30 40 50 60 70 d b s p l half-list 2 1 half-list 2 2 ο +—i 20 30 40 50 60 70 d b s p l figure 1. plots of south african english speakers' mean scores (diamonds) with error bars (2 s.d) for each tygerberg recording cid w-22 full-list and half-list, and the original american english speakers' mean scores (squares) for the american cid w-22 full-list. die suidl-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 wayne j. wilson and selvarani moodley lists for each presentation intensity separately, using within-groups f test analyses at the 1% significance level. results south african english speakers' versus american english speakers' scores figure 1 displays a plot of the sae speakers' mean scores on the tygerberg cid w22 full-lists 1 and 2, and their respective half-lists 1-1, 1-2, 2-1 and 2-2, at the 20, 30, 40, 50, 60 and 70 dbspl presentation intensities. all plots are superimposed on the extrapolated normative data for the ae speaker mean scores averaged over the four american cid w22 full-lists (hirsh et al., 1952). in general, the sae speakers' performance on the tygerberg cid w22 wordlists was poorer than the ae speakers performance on the american cid w22 wordlists at the lower presentation intensities (< 50 dbspl), but was equivalent at the higher presentation intensities (> 40 dbspl). on closer visual inspection, the sae speakers' scores were: more than two standard deviations lower than the ae speakers' mean scores for half-list 2-1 at 20 dbspl. approximately two standard deviations lower for fulllists 1 and 2, and half-lists 1-1 and 1-2 at 20 dbspl. between one and two standard deviations lower for halflist 2-2 at 20 dbspl and for full-list 1, and half-lists 11, 1-2 and 2-1 at 30 dbspl. less than one standard deviation lower for full-list 2 and half-list 2-2 at 30 dbspl and all lists at 40 dbspl. approximately equal for all lists at 50,60 and 70 dbspl. south african english speakers' scores table 1 shows the mean + one standard deviation scores for 15 female, first language sae speakers on the tygerberg cid w22 full-lists 1 and 2, and their respective half-lists 1-1,1-2, 2-1 and 2-2, at the 20,30,40, 50, 60 and 70 dbspl presentation intensities. the t-test for dependent sample results for differences between the sae speaking subjects' full-list scores, conducted separately for each presentation intensity, showed no significant differences (p<0.05). table 2 shows one-way repeated measures anova and tukeys honest significant difference (thsd) test results for differences between the sae speaking subjects half-list scores, conducted separately for each presentation intensity. results showed a significant difference (p<0.05) between the first half of list 1 (1-1), and the second half of list 1 (1-2) and list 2 (2-2) at 40dbspl only. note that a significant (p<0.05) anova result was observed at the 60 dbspl presentation intensity, but the associated thsd analysis showed no significant differences. wilcoxon matched pairs test results for differences between sae speaking subject scores on each pair of adjacent presentation intensities (20-30, 30-40, 40-50, 5060 and 60-70 dbspl), conducted for each full and half-list separately, showed significant differences (p<0.01) for all comparisons, except between 60-70 dbspl for all full and half-lists. f test analysis for differences in variance between sae speaking subjects scores, between presentation intensities (dbspl) within each full and half-list, showed the following significant differences (p<0.01): (20, 30, 40) > 50 > (60, 70) for full-list 1. 30 > (20, 40) > 50 > (60, 70) for full-list 2. (20,30,40,50) > (60,70); and (30,40) > 50 for half-list 1-1. (20, 30, 40) > 50 > (60, 70) for half-list 1-2. (20,30,40,50) > (60,70); and 30 > (20,50) for half-list 2-1. 30 > (20, 40) > (50, 60, 70) for half-list 2-2. f test analysis for differences in variance between sae speaking subjects scores, between each full and half-list, within each presentation intensity, showed the following significant differences (p<0.01): half-list 1-1 > half-list 2-2 at 40 dbspl. half-list 1-1 > (half-list 1-2, half-list 2-2) at 50 dbspl. half-list 1-1 > (half-list 1-2, half-list 2-1) at 60 dbspl. (half-list 1-1, half-list 2-2) > (half-list 1-2, half-list 2-1) at 70 dbspl. whilst these differences have implications for the anova and thsd analyses of the half-list scores above, the differences were not considered to be in excess of those tolerable by the anova (lindman, 1974). discussion the qualitative finding that the sae speakers' scores on the tygerberg cid w22 wordlists were generally worse than those of the original ae speakers' scores on the dbspl 20 30 40 50 60 70 full-list 1 9.2 + 10.8 53.3 + 13.8 80.1 ±10.8 95.2+4.7 98.5 + 2.2 99.1 ±1.8 full-list 2 10.7 ±8.5 51.6 ±21.7 83.6 ± 9.3 95.9 ±4.0 98.7 + 1.6 99.2 ±1.7 ' half-list 1-1 7.7+12.1 47.2 ± 15.9 73.9+15.9 92.8 ±7.4 97.6 ±3.6 98.4+2.9 half-list 1-2 11.2 ±10.7 53.9+16.5 86.4 ±9.3 97.6 + 3.6 99.5 + 1.4 99.7 + 1.0 half-list 2-1 10.1+8.0 45.9 ±21.7 80.5 + 14.3 95.5 ±5.4 99.5 ± 1.4 99.7 ± 1.0 half-list 2-2 12.5 + 10.9 53.1 + 23.9 86.7 ±7.5 96.0 + 3.7 97.9 ± 2.6 98.7 + 2.5 i table 1: south african english speakers' mean +one s.d. speech discrimination scores on the tygerberg cid w22 full-lists 1 and 2, and their respective half-lists 1-1,1-2, 2-1 and 2-2. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) use of the cid w22 as a south african english speech discrimination test 61 american cid w22 wordlists, indicates that the use of the tygerberg sae recording of the cid w22 wordlists with the original american cid w22 wordlist normative data cannot be applied to the sae speaking population without significant modification. specifically, the differences between the sae and ae speaking subjects on their respective cid w22 wordlists occurred at presentation intensities below 50 dbspl. this suggests that both groups were able to hear their respective cid w22 wordlists equally well at the higher presentation intensities, but the american subjects were able to extract more information from their american wordlists at the lower presentation intensities, due most probably to their greater familiarity with ae. these findings are consistent with the previous finding of sae speakers' performance on the australian english nal-ab wordlists (wilson, jones & fridjhon, 1998). analysis of the sae speakers' scores alone showed several useful findings. the finding that the mean scores showed only two significant differences (p<0.05) when compared within presentation intensities between lists (half-list 1-1 scores were lower than those of half-lists 1-2 and 2-2 at the 40 dbspl presentation intensity only) suggests two things. generally, it suggests that the two tygerberg cid w22 wordlists and their four half-lists were of equal difficulty for the sae speaking subjects at all but one of the tested presentation intensities. specifically, it suggests that halflist 1-1 may be a more difficult half-list at 40 dbspl. the possibility that half-list 1-1 could be a problematic list was further supported by the finding that this half-list had a significantly greater variance (p<0.01) than the other halflists at the higher presentation intensities (> 30 dbspl). such interpretations must be approached with caution, however, as whilst the absolute differences in the mean and variance values were statistically significant, their clinical impact is of somewhat less value due to the small size of the absolute differences observed. the sae speakers' scores also showed a significant improvement (p<0.05) with each increase in presentation intensity from 20 to 60 dbspl, within each tygerberg cid w22 full and half-list, as would be generally expected in a /normal performance intensity function. the absence of ' significant differences between the 60 and 70 dbspl presentation intensities provides evidence of the beginning of a ceiling effect at these intensities. table 2: anova and tukeys hsd results for sae speakers' speech discrimination scores on e tygerberg cid w22 wordlists for half-lists 1-1, 1-2, 2-1 and 2-2. dbspl anova thsd 20 p=0.38 30 p=0.18 40 p<0.001 l-l<(l-2,2-2) 50 p=0.06 60 p<0.05 70 p=0.06 the significant decrease (p<0.05) in the sae speakers' score variances with increases in presentation intensity is remarkably similar to previous findings of sae speakers' performance on the australian nal-ab wordlists (wilson, jones and fridjhon, 1998). this finding, again, suggests a more equal ability between subjects to overcome any problems with non-sae english, in this case ae, when given a high enough presentation intensity, and only partly agrees with ostergard's (1983) findings that variability in speech discrimination scores tend to decrease for extreme scores and increase for mid-range scores. minor limitations to this study include the young, adult, female composition of the sample and the lack of control over right versus left ear selection. with no reports, to the authors' knowledge, of significant female/male or right/left ear performance differences in the literature for any of the monosyllabic word tests, these points were not considered to have had any significant impact on this study's findings. a more significant limitation was prerequisite that the sample should be first language sae speakers, and the predominance of tertiary level education. davis (1983) (cited in lutman, 1987) demonstrated a relationship between socio-economic status and type of occupation and performance in speech audiometry, with higher education level implying that a certain level of linguistic competence and even sophistication affects the results. it might therefore be predicted that a more representative sample of the sae speaking population would not have performed as well as the more educated sample used in this study. finally, it must be noted a significant factor affecting any interpretation of speech discrimination results is the large amount of variability inherent in speech audio me trie testing. mendel and danhauer (1997) warn that a margin of 16-20% should be allowed for erroneous scoring alone, because of errors in the scorer's perception. similarly, thornton and raffin (1978), ostergard (1983) and green (1987) state that a single score obtained for a particular wordlist is only an indicator of a range of scores in which the true score is likely to lie. this inherent variability in speech audiometry diminishes its accuracy in all uses generally and the strength of the descriptive results of this study specifically. conclusions the similarities observed between the sae and ae speakers on their respective pre-recorded cid w22 wordlist recordings makes the current south african practice of using the tygerberg sae recording with the original american normative data a valid option for the speech discrimination assessment of sae speakers, under restricted conditions. the tygerberg cid w22 recording with the american normative data is most suitable for use at suprathreshold intensities (> 40 dbspl) where the fewest differences were observed. use at or near threshold (< 50 dbspl) however, where the most differences were observed, should be approached with caution and reliance on these wordlists for site of lesion purposes should be avoided. in view of the generally equivalent performance of the sae speakers within the tygerberg cid w22 full and half-lists (a high "test equivalency"), this recording's general test reliability and validity could be quickly and easily improved by replacing the american normative data with large south african normative data-bases. despite this study's findings, the over-riding need for a die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 wayne j. wilson and selvarani m o o d l e y sae specific speech discrimination test remains. whilst the t y g e r b e r g c i d w 2 2 w o r d l i s t s w i t h t h e a m e r i c a n normative data was shown to have some validity and reliability in sae speakers, significant limitations exist in its use, and the continued use of non-south african tests must be seen as an interim measure only. in view of similarities between the performance of sae and ae speakers on their respective cid w22 wordlists in this study, and similar findings in other wordlists (wipi and n u c h i p s ( m e h l , 1 9 9 2 ) a n d n a l a b w o r d l i s t s (wilson, jones and fridjhon, 1998)), there is now a small but growing body o f literature that can provide some of the groundwork needed to assist in the development of the long awaited sae specific speech discrimination test. r e f e r e n c e l i s t brandy, w.t. (1966). reliability of voice tests of speech discrimination. journal of speech and hearing research, 9: 461-465. brewer, c.c. & resnick, d.m. (1983). a review of tests of speech discrimination. seminars in hearing, 4: 205-220. carhart, r. (1965). problems in the measurement of speech discrimination. archives of otolaryngology, 82(9): 253-260. edgerton, b.j. & danhauer, j.l. (1979). clinical implications of speech discrimination lasting using nonsense stimuli. baltimore: university park press. fuller, h. (1987). equipment for speech audiometry and its calibration. in m. martin (ed.), speech audiometry. london: taylor and francis ltd. green, r. (1987). the uses and misuses of speech audiometry in rehabilitation. in m. martin (ed.), speech audiometry. london: taylor and francis ltd. hall, j.w. (1983). diagnostic applications of speech audiometry. seminars in hearing, 4: 179-204. hirsh, i.j., hallowell, d., silverman, s.r., reynolds, e.g., eldert, e. & benson, r.w. (1952). development of materials for speech audiometry. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 15:321337. kreul, e.j., bell, d.w. & nixon j.c. (1969). factors affecting speech discrimination test difficulty. journal of speech and hearing research, 12(2): 281-287. lehiste, i. & peterson, g.e. (1959). linguistic considerations in the study of speech intelligibility, journal of the acoustic society of america, 31: 280-286. lindman, h.r. (1974). analysis of variance in complex experimental design. san fransisco: w.h. freeman. lutman, m.e. (1987). speech tests in quiet and noise as a measure of auditory processing. in m. martin (ed.) speech audiometry (chapter 3). london: taylor & francis ltd. martin, f.n & forbis n.k. (1978). the present status of audiometric practice: a follow up study. asha, 20: 531-541. mendel, l.l. & danhauer, j.l. (1997). audiological evaluation and management and speech perception assessment. san diego: singular publishing group inc. northern, j.l. & hattler, k.w. (1974). evaluation of four speech discrimination test procedures on hearing impaired patients. journal of auditory research, (suppl): 1-37. ostergard, c.a. (1983). factors influencing the validity and reliability of speech audiometry seminars in hearing, 4: 221240. thorton, a.r. & raffin, m.j. (1978). speech discrimination scores modelled as a binomial variable. journal of speech and hearing research, 21(3): 507-518. wilson, w., jones, b. & fridhon, p. (1998). use of the nal-ab wordlists as a south african english specific speech discrimination test. south african journal of communication disorders, 45: 77-86. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 future trends in language intervention: addressing cultural bias in service delivery* anne van kleeck professor program in communication sciences and disorders department of speech communication university of texas at austin austin, texas 78712 usa abstract in this paper the cultural biases in a widely-used language intervention approach the hanen early language parent program that trains parents to he conversational partners with their language-delayed children, are explored. in many respects this program represents tye best of current clinical practice. it is empirically and theoretically grounded in recent research on parent-child interaction; studies have documented its efficacy; and it is a family-centred approach. and yet, in clinical practice, it does not work with all families. not surprisingly, these families are often from nonmainstream backgrounds. potential reasons for the lack ofeffectiveness with some families become apparent as research on patterns of language socialization in a wide variety ofcultures is reviewed. this review reveals that all of the basic premises of this program rest on culturally relative beliφ and values. specifically considered are cultural variation in (1) aspects of social organization related to interaction, (2) the value of talk, (3) how status is handled in interaction, (4) beliefs about intentionality, and (5) belief about teaching language to children. suggestions for incorporating this information into clinical services with nonmainstream families are offered. opsomming die kulturele bevooroordeling van die "hanen early language parent program" word ondersoek. hierdie program is tans algemeen ingebruik en verteenwoordig die beste in huidige kliniese riglyne. dit is 'n taalopleidingsbenadering wat ouers oplei asgespreksgenote vir hul kinders met vertraagde taalverwerwing. die program is empiries en teoreties gebaseer op onlangse navorsing in ouer-kindinteraksie; verdere studies lewer bewys van die effektiwiteit van die program; en dit is 'n gesinsgesentreerde benadering. kliniesegebruik van die program dui egter daarop dat dit nie opgesinne buite die hoofctroomagtergrond, van toepassing isnie. potensiele redes vir diegebrek aan effektiwiteit met sekere gesinne word uitgelig, wanneer die patrone van taalsosialisasie van verskillende kulture nagegaan word. die gevolgtrekking word gemaak dat die basiese grondbeginsels van hierdie program op kultuur verwante waardes en beginsels berus. kulturele verskille bestaan, veral ten opsigte van (1) aspekte van sosiale organisasie wat verband hou met interaksie, (2) die waarde van gesprek, (3) bantering van status in inter aksie, (4) beginsels ten opsigte van intensionaliteit en (5) waardes aangaande taalopleiding vir kinders. voorstelle word gemaak vir die implimentering^van hierdie inligting in die dienslewering aan nie-hoofstroom gesinne. ι when i entered the field of communication disorders as an undergraduate in 1970, we called ourselves speech pathologists. we had no courses on children's language development or language disorders, indeed, language had not yet entered our realm. in the fall of 1973,1 entered a masters program at columbia university and there met lois bloom. lois had been a practising speech-pathologist for a number of years and was inspired to do doctoral work because her clinical experience had led her to the realization that there was more to children's communication disorders than their difficulties with speech. when i met her, she had just published her pioneering work, language development: form and function in emerging grammar (1970). other than some diary studies done by linguist * this paper was delivered as the sixth p. de v. pienaar memorial lecture held in the senate house, university ofwitwatersrand, on 20 july 1992. parents, it was one of the very first in depth, comprehensive studies of early language development. her work, along with that of psychologist roger brown (1973) among others, was to revolutionize the field of communication disorders by adding language to its purview. the national association of professionals in communication disorders in the united states was renamed in 1977 to reflect this change the american speech and hearing association became the american speechlanguage-hearing association. now, twenty years later, i sometimes find myself in absolute awe at how far we have come in our understanding of children's language development and language disorders. there are now dozens of journals and hundreds of books devoted, in part or full, to scholarship in this area. armed with the vast amount of knowledge spawned by twenty years of vigorous research and clinical practice, we are more effective than ever in assisting children who have difficulty in acquiring that most basic and die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 39, 1992 ® saslha r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 anne van kleeck essential of human capacities language. i like to remind myself from time to time of this rich and rewarding history, of how far we have come, and of how much better we are currently doing. i like to remind myself because it helps balance another feeling that frequently surfaces that of being overwhelmed. overwhelmed because, as we learn more, the task of pulling that knowledge together into a cohesive whole becomes increasingly daunting. and clinical practice that does not at least attempt to deal with the whole, is, in my opinion, completely missing the mark. i feel overwhelmed, too, because as we learn more, we simultaneously become aware of how much more we need to understand. the complexity seems to multiply exponentially. one is never allowed to rest, it seems, with knowledge hard-gained.new issues continually beg attention: and, because we care about providing the very best service delivery, we cannot ignore them. i'd like to focus my talk today on an issue, or rather, a complex set of issues, that began as an uncomfortable stirring for me about three years ago. it is only recently, however, that the faint rumblings of something being amiss in my thinking has worked its way into a more conscious awareness of what might lie at root. let me provide a little background. in 1989,1 was awarded a training grant by the united states department of education to prepare specialists to work with handicapped infants and toddlers and their families. these funds were made available to fill a need created by a federal law passed in 1986, public law 99-457 (now p.l. 101-476), which mandated that states put in place a plan to provide services to families with handicapped children from birth to three years of age. previous legislation (public law 94-142) had required services for children above three years of age. public law 99-457 further specified that services must be familycentered, meaning that families would not only be closely involved in all assessment and intervention, but that they would also determine priorities for their child's services. they would, in effect, be equal team members. to work effectively with families, a clinician must respect individual family structure and interaction patterns. to do so, the need for cultural sensitivity becomes glaringly apparent. as i have worked with my grant project director, ann thomas, in developing course work and in setting up an infant/ toddler assessment team in our university clinic, i have come to realize that implementing family-centered services may be difficult for deeper reasons than the mere fact that change is always difficult. for me, the process of implementing familycentered services has forced into consciousness some points at which the values of our mainstream culture, of our profession, and of family-centered services for nonmainstream families may be in conflict. if we are to take the next step in providing even more efficacious services, we need to grapple openly with these sources of conflict. resolving these conflicts will hopefully result in changes that not only acknowledge the complexity posed by cultural diversity, but begin to offer more concrete guidance for our thinking about language assessment and intervention with children from non-mainstream families. given the multicultural nature of both of our societies, this issue seems as relevant to south africa, in essence if not in details, as it is to the united states. the mainstream cultural values in the united states that are particularly relevant to the discussion at hand include (a) the high value placed on verbal/literate skills, (b) our democratic ideal of equal opportunity for all, beginning with equal access to education and extending to maximizing the potential for upward mobility of all citizens, and (c) the value placed on individual choice. as we translate these cultural values into professional values, we find (a) a high value placed on facilitating the very "best" verbal/literate skills possible in all children we serve, and (b) that our notion of "best" is determined by those skills we know will foster the best chances for academic success, and, by implication, for lifelong success. the introduction of family-centered services addresses the third cultural value of individual choice families should be able to choose not only the kind of services they prefer from all options available, they should also have the final word on the goals to be focussed upon and the procedures that should be used to facilitate reaching those goals. a current, widely used intervention program reflects these values directly. first of all, it rests on the basic premise that one should attempt to get children to be as communicative as possible. the adult-child interaction patterns determined in research to best foster children's communicative attempts and subsequent language development provide the basis of the program. and finally, it is family-centered in that the interaction patterns are directly taught to the language delayed child's parents. the program, the hanen early language parent program, was developed by ayala manolson in toronto, canada (l 985). i'd like to focus on the hanen program to illustrate the pressing need for speech-language pathologists to become aware of the cultural biases it entails. this is not done to denigrate this program we have used it fruitfully in our clinic for years. the program is focussed on exactly because it does, in my mind, represent the best of current clinical practice. a careful analysis of the program's underlying cultural biases, however, uncovers dramatically the need for far more cultural sensitivity in our clinical practice than is presently the norm. the hanen early language parent program is based largely on the clinical work of jim macdonald, who in turn has grounded his ideas in an ever-growing body of basic research on adult-child interaction (e.g., macdonald, 1989). this program works directly with the parents (both mother and father whenever possible) to foster the interaction patterns within the family that will promote communication development. it is aimed at parents of children who are either preverbal or at the earliest stages of verbal development. several sets of parents are brought together in weekly three-hour meetings for eight to ten weeks. program objectives are taught via workbooks, lectures, demonstrations, training videotapes, videotapes of the parents in the group interacting with their own children, group discussion, role playing and homework assignments. to make the current videotapes, all parents enrolled in the program are videotaped at home interacting with their own children at three different junctures once before, once during and once after the program so that specific implementation can be discussed. the interaction strategies are based upon those that research has found ( l ) to best promote language development, and (2) to be lacking or infrequent in the interaction of parents with their language delayed children. the strategies are focussed one at a time, and include, sequentially, observing your child (to accurately assess the child's current communication level), following your child's lead (including talking at his or her level), responding supportively to the child's attempts at communication, fostering the child's turn-taking skills, prompting the child to a higher level of communicative performance, and designing play activities to provide communication opportunities (see girolometto, greenberg, & manolson, 1986, for further program description and a summary of supporting research). when i was first trained in this program by ayala manolson several years ago, i felt that the field of speech-language pathology was finally beginning to implement practices that took into account far more of the complex puzzle of factors that are involved in a child's language development. this direct work the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) futuretrends in language intervention with parents on the interactional foundation for language development was a far cry from the behavior modification programs that permeated the field in the 1970's, when clinician's were busy "flipping pictures and pitching tokens" (constable, 1986, p. 211) in pursuit of that elusive "is verb-ing" form. it also moved us beyond the subsequent application of the semantic and pragmatic revolutions when we were attempting to create meaningful contexts in our therapy rooms, but were not yet fully acknowledging that children are a part of a family system. hanen, and programs like it, have provided a much needed family-centered option for facilitating language development. a systematic clinical tool, it fosters parent-child interaction patterns that encourage children's communicative behavior and, consequently, their communicative development. the hanen program is well grounded both theoretically and empirically, and is often very effective. however, in our clinical experience, it does not work with all families. not surprisingly, these families are often from minority cultures. why doesn't it work? perhaps because many nonmainstream cultural values and beliefs that impact interaction patterns are at odds with those the hanen program attempts to foster. indeed, the empirical and subsequent theoretical underpinnings of the hanen program are based in large part on north american psycholinguistic research that has focussed almost exclusively on the white middle class. this fact is often unmentioned in these studies, a phenomenon ochs andschieffelin (1984) refer to as "the invisible" culture of child language studies. because the cultural biases of these studies have remained largely implicit, we have often assumed they illuminate natural rather than cultural behaviors (ochs & schieffelin, 1984, p. 284). in the following discussion, patterns of language socialization in a wide variety of cultures will be explored.in particular, i will consider cultural relativity regarding ( l ) aspects of social organization related to interaction, (2) the value of talk, (3) how status is handled in interaction, (4) beliefs about intentionality, and (5) beliefs about teaching language to children. as will be seen in this discussion, all of the basic premises of the hanen program rest on culturally relative social organization, beliefs, and values. ι social organization issues related to interaction the hanen program makes two basic asssumptions about social organization that impact on both how and with whom interaction with young children occurs. the first assumption is about caregiving, which impacts "who talks to small children, in what contexts, and about what topics" (schieffelin & eisenberg, 1984, p. 387). the hanen program is a parentprogram. it assumes that the young child's parents are the primary caregivers. however, caregiving arrangements vary in different cultures. in many cultures, siblings are the primary caregiver, particularly after the baby to be cared for has become mobile. this is true, for example, among the western samoans (ochs, 1982), the kikuyu of eastern africa (leiderman & leiderman, 1974), the gusii of kenya (levine & levine, 1966). it is also common in many american subcultures, including hawaiian, native american, black, and mexicanamerican and other hispanic communities (werner, 1984). the hanen program focus on parent interaction skills would clearly be of less value in such cultures. a second assumption of the hanen program related to social organization has to do with turn-taking. the hanen program assumes that interaction with young children will be primarily dyadic, a predominant pattern in mainstream western culture. however,, multiparty interaction dominates in die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 39, 5 many cultures for example, with the basotho (demuth, 1986), the kaluli (schieffelin, 1979), the samoans (ochs, 1982), and mexican-americans (briggs, 1984; eisenberg, 1982). value of talk let's consider the basic cultural value that undoubtedly accounts for the very existence of speech-language pathology the value of talk. to consider different cultural perspectives on the value of talk, i'd like to look at (1) the amount of talk that is valued, (2) the role of talk in teaching in general, (3) the relative value of verbal skills compared to other types of skills, and (4) the role of verbal skills in children's display of knowledge. 1 amount of talk. regarding amount of talk, a fairly high degree of verbosity in children is valued in our mainstream culture. indeed, an entire line of social science research views reticence as a social deficiency (see daly & mccroskey, 1984), and points out the negative perceptions held in our culture toward reticent children and adults alike. for example, as compared to their more talkative peers, reticent children are viewed by their teachers as significantly less likely to do well in all academic areas and less likely to have positive relationships with other students (mccroskey & daly, 1976). indeed, quietness in the classroom may lead to a speech-language pathology referral. crago (1990b) reports of a principal who was asked to compile a list of children who had speech and language problems. of the 90 children in her school, she listed 30 names and next to several of them noted, "does not talk in class" (p. 79). because mainstream adults value children's talk, they work to elicit it during interactions with children. this mainstream value is sometimes held in other cultures as well. bambi schieffelin studied a traditional nonliterate people in papua new guinea, the kaluli, and found that adults in that society also believed it important to elicit speech from children (schieffelin & eisenberg, 1984). the basotho also highly regard children's ability to interact with others. "indeed, teaching one's child how to talk is seen as one of the major responsibilities of mothers, other caregivers, and the community at large" (demuth, 1986, p. 54). a sesotho proverb loosely translated as "a quiet person will perish" captures the importance of verbal ability in this society (demuth, 1986). such a view is clearly not universal, however. in the working class black community studied by heath (1983), the adults did not ask children questions in order to keep the conversation going, nor did they consider children to be appropriate conversational partners. ward (1971) describes her early attempts at data collection in another black community in rural louisiana. "for the first two months of this project attempts to elicit spontaneous speech from the children met with defeat, with or without the tape recorder. the readiness to show off, the constant flow of speech, the mother-child interaction so common in middle-class children were nowhere in evidence. the children appeared to speak as little to their parents as to the investigator. one twenty-eight month male spoke three words in as many months" ( p. 15). quite to the contrary of our mainstream pattern of socializing children to interact with adults, many cultures value quietness in children. many groups believe that children should speak only when spoken to, such as the kipsingis (harkness, 1977) and luo (blount, 1972; 1977) of kenya, western samoans (ochs, 1982), and the tamil of malaysia (williamson, 1979). and this is true not only in nonindustrialized or third world cultures but in many north american sub-cultures that have been studied as well, such as rural louisiana blacks (ward, 1971) and many american indian groups (crago, 1990b; dumont, 1972; john, 1972; 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 anne van kleeck scollon & scollon, 1981). furthermore, children in many groups are often socialised to a generalised attitude regarding the value of silence. for example, the japanese (clancy, 1986; fischer, 1970) and other asian/pacific groups (see cheng, 1989, for a summary), who have immigrated to the united states in vast numbers in the past decade, often view a quiet child as a good child. this value is also held by the evergrowing chicano population in the united states, as well (coles, 19 77). in many native american cultures, children are taught to be silent in the presence of an adult, especially an unfamiliar adult (blanchard, 1983). indeed, navajo mothers view loquaciousness in a child as discourteous, self-centered, and undisciplined (freedman, 1979). the extreme variation in the value of talk even within the american culture is dramatically illustrated in a study conducted by birdwhistell (1974). he compared the median amount of talk per day in philadelphia jewish homes and in pennsylvania dutch homes (an agricultural group that has eschewed modern technology). the jewish families talked between six and twelve hours a day; the pennsylvania dutch, for two and one half minutes! from a rather different perspective, the scollons provide an example of the extreme differences in cultural expectations regarding children's talk. among the northern canadian athabaskans they studied, "children who do not begin to speak until five years of age or older are interpreted as growing up respectfully, not as being language-delayed'" (scollon & scollon, 1981, p. 134). indeed, our profession's very definition of a learning disability is stood upon its head in the comment made by an inuit teacher in martha crago's study. one of crago's research subjects was a very verbal little boy, whom she considered to be very bright. crago described this child to an inuit teacher and asked her reaction. the teacher said, "do you think he might have a learning problem? some of these children who do not have such high intelligence have trouble stopping themselves. they don't know when to stop talking" (crago, 1990b, p. 80). of course, social values associated with the talkative-reticence continuum are not as clear cut as this discussion might imply. the scollons (1981) discuss how the degree of volubility and degree of intimacy are related in an opposite fashion in north american mainstream and native athabaskan cultures. for mainstream speakers, they suggest, volubility is associated with social distance and taciturnity is acceptable in intimate relationships (that talk is not needed may indeed be a sign of intimacy). for the athabaskans, the opposite is true. they can be very talkative when they know each other well, but are quite reticent with people they have just met. 2 how teaching is accomplished. because our mainstream culture values talk, teaching is often accomplished with a great deal of talk. heath (1989) describes how mainstream adults engage in frequent verbal explanation with children. they intervene in a task being taught to offer step-by-step explanations, and children are afterwards often asked to recount the task verbally. such a teaching style is typical of what hall (1976) refers to as "low-context cultures". in contrast, "highcontext" cultures rely much more heavily on the physical context, and hence nonverbal contextual cues, to convey information. learning is accomplished primarily by observation; teaching by demonstration. westby and rouse (1985) note that hispanic and native american groups in the united states operate much more as high-context cultures in comparison to the anglo mainstream culture. children in lowcontext cultures are encouraged to ask questions; those in more high-context cultures are not. in specific studies, ward found that children's information-seeking questions were ignored among the rural louisiana blacks she studied (1971), and the inuit children studied by crago were also discouraged from asking questions (1990). 3 skills valued. heath (1989) ties a tendency to teach by demonstration with "societies that marshal children's efforts toward spatial, kinesthetic, musical, and interpersonal competence as prior or at least of equal merit with linguistic competence" (p. 342). so, once again, verbal skills are not always valued above all else, as they often seem to be in our mainstream culture. 4 display of knowledge. a final aspect of the value of talk within a given culture has to do with how knowledge is displayed, and by whom. scollon and scollon (1981) discuss the dimensions of spectatorship/exhibitionism in relation to dominant and subordinate roles. they explain how in our mainstream culture, the dominant person is the spectator and the subordinate person the exhibitionist. as such, the notion of display of knowledge is related to how status is handled in a culture, a topic i will discuss in the next section. in other cultures, such as the athabaskans the scollons studied, the dominant person is the exhibitionist rather than the spectator. in our mainstream culture, adults often elicit verbal displays of knowledge from their children. they often do this by asking children questions to which they, the adults, already have the answers. the children are nonetheless expected to perform their "exhibitionist" role and respond to these known-information or, as they are sometimes called, "test" questions. this verbal display of knowledge figures predominantly in the school setting as well. in many other cultures, requesting children's verbal display of knowledge is either not used or is used for different purposes. heath (1983) found that the adults in the black working class community she studied only rarely asked known information questions. when they did, the purpose was to chastise the child. heath (1989) summarizes several unpublished studies of mexicanamerican showing that adults refrain from asking children known-information questions, except in teasing exchanges. other groups not prone to using test questions in interaction with their children include the inuit studied by crago (1990b) and the rural louisiana blacks studied by ward (1971). ward, in fact notes, "such children are not expected to exhibit any range of manners, skills, or special knowledge" (p. 53). in these various nonmainstream cultures, children are expected to be spectators and learn by observation. how status is handled in interactions i in the hanen program we ask parents to respond to all [the child's attempts to initiate interaction, to follow the child's lead to facilitate conversation, to use simple language and to expand the child's communicative attempts so that the child will have the opportunity to learn from responses, and to iise questions to confirm or clarify. each of these suggestions rests upon underlying assumptions assumptions not always shared by other cultural groups. let's consider them by looking at how various cultures deal with (1) who initiates adultchild interaction, (2) who directs adult-child interaction, (3) who adapts to whom, and (4) who carries the burden of understanding. 1 who initiates adult-child interaction. in mainstream culture, while adults are certainly allowed to initiate interaction with children, children are also encouraged to initiate interaction with adults. we witness this primarily by observing how responsive adults are to children's initiations, intentional or otherwise. indeed, researchers have written entire articles on the positive impact of caregiver contingent social responsiveness to infants on their children's subsequent the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) futuretrends in language intervention 7 subsequent acquisition of communicative competence (e.g., dunst, lowe, & bartholomew, 1989-90; goldberg, 1977). but once again, this is clearly not a universal phenomenon. the rural blacks studied by ward (1971) paid more attention to a child when the child was not talking than when she or he was. in the black community studied by heath (1983), the adults did not tend to ask many questions in order to keep an interaction going with a child. indeed, these minority cultures and others, such as mexican-americans (discussed by heath, 1989), do not believe that a child should initiate a conversation with an adult. some cultures, such as the japanese, will even anticipate a child's needs in order to foster passivity (caudill & weinstein, 1969). 2 who directs the interaction. it is often the case that the person who initiates an interaction also directs it. schegloff (1972), for example, found that the person who speaks first in a conversation also tends to control the topic. likewise, in mainstream parent-child interaction, the parent follows the child's lead. in other words, the adult talks about what the child is doing or saying. snow refers to this as semantic contingency, and reviews numerous studies providing evidence that this interaction strategy facilitates language acquisition (1979,1981). the temporal characteristics of contingent responses are also important, as has been demonstrated in work with mother-infant dyads. numerous studies have found that most maternal responses to infant vocalizations occur within one second (beebe, jaffe, feldstein, mays, & alson, 1985; schaffer,collis&parsons, 1977;stella-prorok, 1983)andthat response delays of longer than one second interfere with the infant's perception of contingent relationships (millar, 1972; millar & watson, 1979;ramey&ourth, 1971; todd & palmer, 1968). temporal delays in adults' responses have also been found to completely disrupt conversations with 2-year-olds, because the children stopped responding (stella-prorok, 1983). obviously, both semantic contingency and such a quick response requires careful attention to the infant by the adult.1 other cultures do not allow children to direct topics (e.g., rural louisiana blacks studied by ward, 1971), and may believe it is the adult's role to issue directives and the child's role to obey them (louisiana blacks, the inuits studied by crago, 1990b). in many societies, 'parent-child interaction is not characterized by adult semantic contingency. this is true of the westehi samoans (ochs] & schieffelin, 1984), the kaluli (schieffelin & eisenberg, 1984); the gusii of kenya (levine, 1977), and rural louisiana and carolina blacks in the united states (heath, 1983; ward, 1971). 3 who adapts to whom: assuming the perspective of the child in making semantically, contingent responses is one way in which mainstream parents make rather extensive accommodations to the child. such accommodations are in part determined by how status is handled in a culture. in our mainstream culture, persons of higher status are expected to adapt to those of lower status hence, adults adapt to children. as ochs and schieffelin (1984) note, it is a pattern that extends beyond adult-child interactions and is also observed in widespread material accommodations, such as baby clothes and 1 the scallons (1981) discuss one instance of timing differences in interaction that between the athabaskans they studied and north american mainstream culture. the pause time between turns in conversation is slightly longer for athabaskans, although perhaps only a half second longer. the impact on interaction between these two groups, however, is dramatic. the mainstream speakers give adequate pauses for the other to take a turn within their own culture, but it is not long enough for the athabaskan culture. hence, the mainstream speakers do all the talking and the athabaskan never gets a chance. food, and miniaturization of furniture and toys. additional common behavioral accommodations include "baby-proofing" a home, and putting the baby in a quiet place to facilitate sleep. these scholars suggest that "these accommodations reflect a discomfort with the competence differential between adult and child" (p. 287). in interaction, one way that this competence gap is reduced is for the adult to simplify his or her speech to better match the lesser verbal competence of the child. this simplified speech is known by many names "motherese" (newport, gleitman, & gleitman, 1977), "baby talk register" (e.g., brown, 1977), and "child-directed talk (cdt)" (van kleeck & carpenter, 1980) or "child-directed speech (cds)" (snow, 1986). this simplified, reduced, redundant, acoustically distinct input to young children has been extremely well-documented in a variety of middle-class industrialized cultures (see snow, 1986, for a review that relates this simplified register to language acquisition). fischer (1970) notes that the japanese parents seem even more disposed to these speech simplifications than mainstream americans. but such modifications are not solely found in modern, industrialized societies. they have been found to characterize parent-child interactions among the kipsingis in kenya (harkness, 1977) and the kwara'ae of the solomon islands (watson-gegeo & gegeo, 1986a & b). in other cultures, persons of lower status are expected to adapt to those of higher status. this particular cultural value may be manifested in interactions with infants and young children by a lack of child-directed talk simplifications on the part of adults. this is the case with the western samoans studied by ochs (1982). heath also found a lack of such adult adaptation in the working-class black community she studied (heath, 1983). it is not simply the case that cdt modifications are either present or absent in most cultures. schieffelin and ochs (1986) discuss how there is a continuum'of accommodation both across and within cultures. within cultures, variability in the existence or degree of cdt modifications may occur as a function of who is talking to the child, the reason for the interaction, the setting, and the development level of the child. in mainstream american culture, some studies have documented that fathers and siblings do not simplify their talk to babies as much as mothers do (e.g., andersen & kekelis, 1986; bellinger & gleason, 1982;gleason&greif; 1983;mannle&tomasello, 1987). a study by gollinkoff and ames (1979), however, found that fathers did adjust their speech as much as mothers, although they produced half as many utterances and took fewer turns in a free play situation. in japan, mothers simplify far more in private than they do in public (clancy, 1986). the basotho use such modifications when their intent is to amuse or pacify the infant or young child, but they do not use them when talking seriously or reprimanding (demuth, 1986). the luo make no such modifications from birth until about 9 months of age, then make them extensively until the baby's first word at about 14 months. after 14 months, they are used to get attention, but not after attention has been gained (blount 1972). 4 who carries the burden of understanding. lacking an adult facility with the linguistic system, children are frequently difficult to understand. as with other accommodations, mainstream adults have strategies to attempt to deal with an unintelligible or partially intelligible child. they may ask clarification questions (e.g., sachs, 1983) or they may expand the child's utterances. brown (1977) suggests that expansions are communication checks that in effect ask the child, "is this what you mean by what you just said?" (p. 13). wells (1982) offers a different interpretation of expansions. wells found die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 39,1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 anne van kleeck that expansions in mother-child interaction were contextually determined. they rarely occurred when the dyad was alone, but were frequent when the mother-child dyad was in a group. the expansions seemed to serve as a way of explaining to others outside the mother-child dyad what the child might be intending to say. in either case, it is clearly the adult who carries the burden for making the child understood. ochs and schieffelin (1984) explain how status is handled differently among the western samoans. here persons of lesser status have the responsibility for making themselves clear. as such, adults do not expand children's utterances. a lack of expansions in adults' speech to children has been found in other cultures, as well, including the rural louisiana blacks studied by ward (1971) and the kipsingis of kenya studied by harkness (1977). interestingly, while mothers in ward's study did not imitate or expand the speech of their children, they did repeat and expand their own speech. beliefs about intentionality some components of the hanen program rest on cultural assumptions about intentionality. parents in the program are asked to freely interpret a child's intended meanings and provide labels for preverbal children and expansions for children in the early stages of language development. let's look at the assumptions underlying these suggestions regarding (1) other intentions and (2) when intentionality begins. 1 others intentions. many behaviors characteristic of mother-child interaction in american mainstream culture make it appear that adults in our culture believe that one person can interpret another's intentions. mothers often interpret internal states of infants as they engage in "conversation" with them (e.g., snow, 1977). with older children, nonconventional forms are often accepted as words, and mothers often provide the label they believe the child is intending to produce. indeed, this occurs with many unintelligible or partially intelligible utterances (ochs, 1982). not all cultures however, believe that one can know what another thinks or feels. ochs and schieffelin (1984) discuss this as underlying the lack of adult interpretation or expansions of children's communicative attempts among the kaluli. it is interesting to note that this same adult behavior, a lack of expansions, can stem from two quite different sources. recall that the samoans, also discussed by ochs and schieffelin, did not expand children's utterances for status reasons. 2 when intentionality begins. cultures also vary regarding when they begin treating children as intentional. in our mainstream culture, we treat children as intentional from birth. this is done by engaging the infant in "conversational dialogue", often by interpreting prelinguistic behaviors (e.g., lock, 1981; ryan, 1974; shorter, 1978). such social "conversations" are common from birth among the kwara'ae as well. however, they are clearly not universal. among the northern new mexico hispanics studied by briggs (1984), a child is not treated as intentional until she or he is one year of age. a similar type of pattern among mexican-american families in california was noted by eisenberg (1982). in western samoa, sibling caregivers begin treating babies as intentional once the baby is mobile (ochs, 1988). neither the kaluli of papua new guinea (ochs & schieffelin, 1984), the luo (blount, 1972), nor american blacks studied by heath (1983) direct much input to prelinguistic babies. navajo mothers are silent with their infants (freedman, 1979). language teaching beliefs the hanen program clearly espouses engaging young prelinguistic and beginning language users in a conversation-like exchange which is structured so that the child can be an "equal" participant. the goal is to get children to communicate as frequently as possible, but directive techniques for eliciting language are strongly discouraged.fostering equal participation in a conversation as a method of facilitating language acquisition is also a culturally determined phenomenon. in addition to conversation, other culturally sanctioned methods of facilitating language acquisition include direct teaching by eliciting imitations from children and a "look and listen" approach in which children are expected to learn by observation. direct teaching. the use of direct teaching as a general method of facilitating language acquisition is actually a fairly widespread phenomenon, found both in american minority cultures and a variety of other cultures. it is also not entirely unheard of in american mainstream culture. for example, this method is used to train young children to use politeness routines such as "please" and "thank you", general greetings such as "hello, how are you?" and "i'm fine", and holiday routines, such as "trick or treat" (gleason & weintraub, 1977). snow (1977) noted that adults began to expect children not only to respond, but to make appropriate responses by the time the children were 18 months old. appropriate responses were elicited by the mothers via direct instruction in three contexts soliciting the names of people, correcting mislabelings, and eliciting polite forms. indeed, politeness prompts have been found in other american cultures, including the working-class americans in south baltimore (miller, 1982) and mexicanamericans in california (eisenberg, 1982). in mainstream culture, direct teaching is mainly restricted to politeness routines. in many other cultures, it is used far more frequently. the literature abounds with examples of groups found to rely heavily on direct teaching devices to socialize children into the language of their culture. of non-american cultures, direct teaching has been reported of the kaluli after the child produces his or her first word (schieffelin & eisenberg, 1984), the samoans (ochs, 1982), for sons at about 4 years of age among the kugu-nganychara, an australian aboriginal tribe (von stunner, 1980), the kwara'ae (watson-gegeo & gegeo, 1986a & b), the wolof of senegal (wills, 19 77), and the wogeo of new guinea (hogbin, 1946). demuth (1986) reports on the very prominent use of direct teaching prompts to young children among the basotho. in america, direct teaching was found to be used extensively by the working class families in south baltimore studied by peggy miller (1982). it was also reported to be a device used among the northern new mexico1 hispanics studied by briggs (1984) once children reached about 14 months of age and mexican immigrant families in california studied by eisenberg (1982). eisenberg (1990) suggests that prompting routines, in which the caregiver tells the child what to say to someone else, are most common when conversations are multiparty. while this is true for the kaluli, kwara'ae, and mexican-americans studied, in miller's study of working-class families in south baltimore, interactions were mostly dyadic. as widespread as this practice of encouraging children to imitate language is, there are some cultures in which the imitation of others' speech is actively discouraged, as in italy (ervin-tripp & mitchell-kernan, 1977). interestingly, although the hanen program explicitly discourages parents .from using direct prompts to get children to talk, they strongly recommend that parents imitate their children, both verbally and nonverbally. research has documented that the amount that a child imitates is correlated with the amount of imitating his or her parent does. that is, highly imitative parents have highly imitative, more linguistically advanced children (folger & chapman, 1978; masur, 1989; the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) futuretrends in language intervention 9 snow, 1989). these findings are presaged by fortes study of the tallensi in ghana (1938), who noted that adults frequently imitated the babblings of children and expected the children to repeat in return. blount (1972) noted that mutual imitation was characteristic of luo parent-child interaction when the children were between 9 and 14 months old. so, while not advocating direct teaching, the hanen program accomplishes basically the same thing indirectly by asking parents to imitate their children. it gets children to imitate. observation. a number of cultures believe that language, as with other skills, is acquired by observation. as such, the ability to learn language is not tied to any overt production of speech. this has been reported of the inuit in northern canada studied by crago (1990b), the rural louisiana blacks studied by ward (1971), and the athabaskans studied by the scollons (1981). multiple approaches. while some cultures may exhibit a strong tendency to emphasize one of the foregoing methods of teaching language to children, the kwara'ae seem to believe that the best and fastest way to teach children language is multifaceted. in their culture we witness, first of all, lots of talk to children: talk that contains many simplifications. the kwara'ae encourage child initiations. they also use routines and direct teaching to facilitate language development. language socialization research: implications for slps using the hanen program rees and gerber (1992) provide an apt conclusion to the foregoing discussion of how cultural beliefs and values shape interaction in the opening sentence of their recent article. "for practitioners whose fascination with language has led them to language disorders as a field of study and a professional commitment, language in use has turned out to be of bottomless complexity and, yet, of central importance" (p.15). of central importance, indeed! the task before us now is to apply this growing body of information in a manner that makes our clinical interventions ever more effective. several ways in which we might begin to do so are 'offered below. \ become aware of our• own cultural biases. first and foremost, professionals must begin to consciously recognize their own "invisible culturej' (crago, 1992) and hence their own cultural biases. cheng (|1989) states that "it is of utmost importance that service providers make a critical examination of their world view, values, beliefs, way of life, communication style, learning style, cognitive study, and personal life history" (pp. 7-8). rice (1986) provides the concrete example of tolerance for dirt. while this may on the surface seem irrelevant to implementing the hanen program, one of my graduate classes had a long discussion about how they felt about visiting a family who had very different cleanliness standards from their own. as a visiting clinician recounted her uncontrollable squeal in reaction to a rat on the floor in one home, one student openly admitted that she simply could not tolerate such a situation. obviously there are many, many more biases we unconsciously harbor that could potentially interfere with our effective service delivery as professionals. uncovering them is not a simple task. just as children are socialized into a given culture, professionals are likewise socialized into a professional culture. and in many ways, asking professionals to become self-aware is in itself a cultural violation, since it has clearly not been a part of their professional socialization. "what is missing, or else minimally encountered, in both graduate programs and staff development training is a focus upon the person who is the helper" (krill, 1990, p. 12). and yet, the need for selfawareness among professionals has become glaringly apparent to me as we have begun working with families of handicapped infants and toddlers. 2 become cross-cultural communicators. much of the research i have reviewed poignantly points out the very different ways in which interaction may be structured and interpreted, even among people who speak the same language. to be effective, clinicians clearly need to be aware of these potential differences. in massively polyglot societies such as the united states, it is clearly unreasonable to expect clinicians and educators to be fluent in vast numbers of languages. in working with families who speak different languages from ourselves, knowledge about other cultural communication patterns would help by at least attenuating potential cultural barriers to communication. 3 -learn about each family's communication patterns. a danger inherent in the type of review provided here is that it can rather ironically lead to stereotyping members of various cultural groups. ochs (1986) reminds us that "cross-cultural differences turn out to be differences in context and/or frequency of occurrence" (p. 10). furthermore, the caveat to remember that cultural trends do not ever define all members of a culture are voiced over and over again in the literature. we are asked to pay heed to the fact that no culture is a monolith (schieffelin & ochs, 1986b); that "variation exists in communities, families, and individuals" (crago, 1992, p. 30); that there is "tremendous individual variation within each group" (miller, 1982, p. 14); "that what may appear to the outsider as one cultural group (e.g., asians) with one set of practices may, in fact, have numerous subgroups with substantially different belief systems, political and economic histories, and cultural patterns of communication" (crago & cole, 1991, p. 111). perhaps this warning is best summed up by rice who says, "each child's family constitutes a cultural entity" (1986, p. 267). how do we go about ascertaining the structure of communication within any particular family? westby (1990) offers many suggestions on ethnographic interviewing as one technique. obviously, observing the family interacting is also invaluable (e.g., andrews & andrews, 1990). 4 culturally situate the notion of a disorder or delay. saville-troike notes that "any study of language pathologies outside one's own speech community must include culturespecific information on what is considered 'normal' and 'aberrant' performance with the other group" (1982, p. 9). for example, matsuda (1989) discusses how in most asian cultures, only physical disabilities are considered to be disabilities. in this case, a serious delay in other domains of development may be denied, and the sensitive clinician would need to be aware of such a tendency to deal with families holding such beliefs. from a quite different perspective, there may sometimes be a tendency to see a disorder where none in fact exists if the communication patterns of the culture are taken into account. we might, for example, diagnose as languagedelayed a reticent native american or asian-american child (crago & cole, 1991). however, here we must be extremely careful not to go to the opposite extreme and "conclude that there is no disorder when one indeed exists" (harris, 1985, p. 43). 5 make instruction culturally congruent. andrews and andrews (1990) suggest that intervention with families will be efficacious only if they are fitted to current family structure. supporting, if somewhat tangential, research can be found in school studies. when school instruction is made culturally congruent, children from minority cultures are shown to be die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykins, vol. 39,1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 anne van kleeck competent learners and communicators (e.g., erickson & mohatt, 1982; heath, 1982; hu-pei au, 1980; kawakami & hu-pei au, 1986; vogt, jordan & tharp, 1987). and, the reverse is also true penalization results from discontinuities between home and school cultures (e.g., boggs, 1985; durnate & ochs, 1988; erickson, 1987; heath, 1983; philips, 1983; tharp et al., 1984). 6 offer parents option to learn code-switching. schools are institutions that reflect and reinforce values and beliefs of the mainstream culture. we know that mainstream interaction patterns best prepare children for the school environment, and best ensure their continued academic success (see heath, 1983, for example). while we should certainly begin our interventions by gearing them to current family structure, we would likely be trying to move the family in a clearly specified direction toward mainstream patterns of interaction. a failure to do so might diminish the nonmainstream child's chances for academic success an outcome that would clearly violate our own basic equal opportunity cultural value. how can we reconcile this dilemma? we want on the one hand to respect the family as it is. that entails accepting all of their values and beliefs, including those that impact upon communication patterns. and yet, the child's chances for success are also of paramount importance to us. our only choice, it seems to me, is to discuss this matter with parents in an open and honest manner. it would then be their choice, not ours, regarding whether or not they wished to pursue a parent training such as that offered by the hanen program. the goals of the program, including its cultural biases, would then be explicitly chosen or eschewed by the family rather than unconsciously imposed by the professional. if our mainstream patterns of interaction with young children are simply too much at odds with the values of the family we are serving, we might alternatively suggest that mainstream interaction patterns be taught and practised in a very constrained context. the final decision on whether or not they wish to become bi-cultural, however, must rest with the family and must also be genuinely supported by the clinician. 7 have clinician act as language socializer. there may be instances where the family is unwilling or perhaps unable to socialize their own child in a manner that will facilitate success in our mainstream culture. a program such as hanen would obviously be an inappropriate intervention for them. in such cases, however, the clinician could offer the option of serving as a representative of the dominant culture. as such, the clinician would work directly with the child to foster the child's ability to code-switch from nonmainstream to mainstream patterns of interaction (rice, 1986). crago (1992), however, suggests that the code-switching option would work more successfully when children's metalinguistic skills are better developed, which generally occurs in the middle childhood years. crago's caution seems relevant if the clinician were to teach such code-switching to the child as a consciously implemented set of rules. it seems entirely possible, however, to foster code-switching skills in much younger children if conscious approaches are not employed. as such, the clinician could use a mainstream pattern of interaction with the child, who would gradually internalize this new interactional system, even though it differed from patterns of interaction with her or his parents. 8 inform teachers about their children's culturally determined patterns of interaction. as the classroom collaborative model of service delivery continues to gain in popularity in the united states, the opportunities for slps to impact a child's communicative experiences in the classroom increase exponentially. clearly, children will benefit if teachers are made aware of the culturally determined differences in their interaction patterns. this is an obvious role the slp can serve. conclusion as westby (1990) noted, "traditional educational and therapeutic intervention models have not been effective with minority populations" (p. 110). and yet, in the next decade, as much as one-third of the caseload of audiologists and slps in the united states will be children from black, hispanic, and native american cultures (cole, 1989; crago, 1990a; shewan, 1988). if our services are to be effective, we must find ways to make them more culturally sensitive. the foregoing discussion of the hanen early language parent program offers but one example of the kinds of cultural biases to be found in even the very best of current clinical interventions. just as we have much to leam from research on language socialization in nonmainstream cultures, so toocan we learn from the nonmainstream families we serve. to quote the mexican novelist carlos fuentes from his recent cultural history of spain and latin america: people and their cultures perish in isolation, but they are born and reborn in contact with other men and women, with men and women of another culture, another creed, another race. if we do not recognize our humanity in others, we shall not recognize it in ourselves. from the buried mirror by carlos fuentes (1992) references anderson, e. &kekelis, l. (1986). siblings' contribution to the interactional competence of blind children. (quarterly newsletter of the laboratory of comparative human cognition. andrews, j. & andrews, m. (1990). family based treatment in communicative disorders. sandwich, illinois: janelle publications. beebe, b., jaffe, j., feldstein, s., mays, k. & alson, d. (1985). matching timing: the application of an adult dialogue model to motherinfant vocal and kinesic interactions. in t. field, (ed)., infant social perception. norwood, nj: ablex. bellinger, d. & gleason, j. (1982). sex differences in parental directives to young children. sex roles, 8, 1123-1139. birdwhistell, r. (1974). the language of the body: the natural environment. in a. silverstein, (ed)., human communication: theoretical explorations. new york: john wiley. blanchard, e. (1983). the growth and development of american indian children and alaskan native children. in g. powell, (ed)., the psychological development ofminority group children. new york: brunner/mazel. ι bloom, l. (19 70). language development: form and function in emerging grammar. cambridge, ma: mit press. | blount, b. (1972). aspects of luo socialization. language in society, 1, 235-248. blount, b. (1977). ethnography and caretaker-child interaction. in c. snow, & c. ferguson, (eds)., talking to children: language input and acquisition. cambridge: cambridge university press. boggs, s. (1985). speaking, relating, and learning: a study of hawaiian children at home and at school. norwood, nj: ablex. briggs, c. (1984). learning how to ask: native meta communicative competence and the incompetence of field workers. language in society, 13, 1-28. brown, r. (1973). a first language: the early stages. cambridge, ma: harvard university press. brown, r. (1977). introduction. in c. snow, & c. ferguson, (eds)., talking to children: language input and acquisition. cambridge: cambridge university press. caudill, w. & weinstein, η. (1969). maternal care and infant behavior in japan and america. psychiatry, 32, 12-43. cheng, l. (1989). service delivery to asian/pacific lep children: a cross-cultural framework. topics in language disorders, 9(3), 114. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) future trends in language intervention clancy. in ρ (1986) the acquisition of communicative style in japanese. β s c h i e f f e l i n , & e. ochs, (eds)., language socialization across ires c a m b r i d g e : cambridge university press. c o k e (1989). ε pluribus unum pluribus: multicultural imperatives for the 1990s and beyond. asha, 31 65-70. coles r (1977)· eskimos, chicanes, indians. boston: little, brown, η ^tahle c f 1986) the application of scripts in the organization of language intervention contexts. in k. nelson, (ed)., event knowledge: structure and function in development. hillsdale, nj: lawrence erlbaum. . crago, m. (1990a). professional gatekeeping: the multicultural, multilingual challenge. communique, 10-13. crago m. (1990b). development of communicative competence in inuit children: implications for speech-language pathology. journal of childhood communication disorders, 13 (1), 73-83. crago, m. (1992). ethnography and language socialization: a crosscultural perspective. topics in language disorders, 12 (3), 28-39. crago, m. & cole, e. (1991). using ethnography to bring children's communicative and cultural worlds into focus. in t. gallagher, (ed)., pragmatics of language: clinical practice issues. san diego: singular. daly, j. & mccroskey, j. (1984). avoiding communication: shgness, reticence, and communication apprehension. beverly hills, ca: sage publications. demuth, k. (1986). prompting routines in the language socialization of basotho children. in b. schieffelin, & e. ochs, (eds)., language socialization across cultures.. cambridge: cambridge university press. dumont, r. (1972). learning english and how to be silent: studies in sioux and cherokee classrooms. in c. cazden, d. hymes, & v. john, (eds)., functions of language in the classroom. ny: teachers college press. dunst, c., lowe, l. & bartholomew, p. (1989-1990). contingent social responsiveness, family ecology, and infant communicative com-, petence. national student speech language hearing association journal, 17, 39-49. duranti, a. & ochs, e. (1988). literacy instruction in a samoan village. in e. ochs, (ed)., culture and language development: language acquisition and language socialization in a samoan village. cambridge: cambridge university press. eisenberg, a. (1982). language development in cultural perspective: talk in three mexican homes. unpublished ph.d. dissertation. university of california, berkeley. erickson, f. (1987). transformation and school success: the politics and culture of educational achievement. anthropology and educational quarterly, 18, 335-357. erickson, f. & mohatt, g. (1982). cultural organization of participation structures in two classrooms of indian students. in g. spindler, (ed)., doing the ethnography of schooling: educational anthropology in action. orlando, fli: holt, rinehart, & winston. ervin-tripp, s. & mitchell-kernan, c. (1977). child discourse. ny: academic press. j fischer, j. (1970). linguistic socialization: japan and the united states. in r. hill, & r. konig,j(eds)., families in east and west. the hague: mouton. ] folger, j. & chapman, r. (1978).;apragmaticanalysisofspontaneous imitations. journal of child language, 5, 25-38. fortes, m. (1938). social and psychological aspects of education in taleland, africa, 11 (4), 14-74. freedman, d. (1979, january). ethnic differences in babies. human nature magazine, 36-43. fuentes, c. (1992). the buried mirror: reflections on spain and the new world. boston: houghton mifflin. girolametto, l., greenberg, j. & manolson, a. (198.6). developing dialogue skills: the hanen early language parent program. seminars in speech and language, 7 (4), 367-381. gleason, j. & greif, e. (1983). men's speech to young children. in b. thorne, c. kramerae, & n. henley, (eds)., language, gender, and society. rowley, ma: newbury house. gleason, j. & weintraub, s. (1976). the acquisition of routines in child language. language in society, 5, 129-136. goldberg, s. (1977). social competence in infancy: a model of parentinfant interaction. merrill-palmer quarterly, 23, 163-177. golinkoff, r. & ames, g. (1979). a comparison of fathers' and mothers' speech to their young children. child development, 50, 28 32. hall, e. (1976). beyond culture. new york: anchor press/doubleday. harkness, s. (1977). aspects of social environment and first language acquisition in rural africa. in c. snow, &c. ferguson, (eds)., talk11 ing to children: language input and acquisition. cambridge: cambridge university press. harris, g. (1985). considerations in assessing english language performance of native american children. topics in language disorders, 5 (4), 42-52. heath, s. b. (1982). ethnography in education: defining the essentials. in p. gilmore, & a. glatthorn, (eds)., children in and out of school: ethnography and education. washington, d.c.: center for applied linguistics. heath, s. b. (1983). ways with words:language, life, and work in communities and classrooms. cambridge: cambridge university press. heath, s. b. (1989). the learner as cultural member. in m. rice, & r. schiefelbusch, (eds)., the teachability of language. baltimore: paul brookes. hogbin, h. (1946). a new guinea childhood: from weaning till the eighth year in wogeo. oceania, 16 (4), 275-296. hu-pei au, κ. (1980). participation structures in a reading lesson with hawaiian children: analysis of culturally appropriate instructional events. anthropology and education quarterly, 11, 91-115. john, v. (1972). styles of learning-styles of teaching: reflections on the education of navajo children. in c. cazden, d. hymes, & v. john, (eds)., functions of language in the classroom. ny: teachers college press. kawakami, α., & hu-pei au, k. (1986). encouraging reading and language development in cultural minority children. topics in language disorders, 6 (2), 71-80. krill, d. (1990). practice wisdom: a guide for helping professionals. newbury park, ca: sage. leiderman, p. & leiderman, g. (1974). affective and cognitive consequences of polymatric care in the east african highlands. in a. pick, (ed)., minnesota symposia on child psychology, vol. 8. london: oxford university press. levine, r. (1977). child rearing as cultural adaptation. in p. liederman, s. tulkin, & a. rosenfeld, (eds)., culture and infancg: variations in the human experience. orlando, fl: academic press. levine, r. & levine, b. (1966). ngansango: a gusii communitg in kngsna. new york: john wiley & sons. lock, a. (1981). the guided reinvention of language. london: academic press. macdonald, j. (1989). becoming partners: from plag to conversation. chicago: riverside publishing. mannle, s. & tomasello, m. (1987). fathers, siblings, and the bridge hypothesis. in k. nelson, & a. van kleeck, (eds)., children's language, vol. 6. hillsdale, nj: lawrence erlbaum. manolson, a. (1985). it takes two to talk: a hanen earlg language parent guide hook (second revision). toronto: hanen early language resource center. masur, ε. (1989). individual and dyadic patterns of imitation: cognitive and social aspects. in g. speidel, & k. nelson, (eds)., the mang faces of imitation in language learning. new york: springer-verlag. matsuda, m. (1989). working with asian parents: some communication strategies. topics in language disorders, 9 (3), 45-53. mccroskey, j. & daly, j. (1976). teachers' expectations of the communication apprehensive child in the elementary school. human communication research, 3 (1), 67-72. millar, w. (19 72). a study of operant conditioning under delayed reinforcement in early infancy. monographs of the societg for research in child development, 37, (2, serial no. 147). millar, w. & watson, j. (1979). the effect of delayed feedback on infant learning re-examined. child development, 50, 747-751. miller, p. (1982). amg, wendg, and beth: learning language in south baltimore. austin, tx: university of texas press. newport, e., gleitman, h. & gleitman, l. (1977). mother, i'd rather do it myself: some effects and non-effects of maternal speech style. in c. snow, & c. ferguson, (eds)., talking to children: language input and acquisition. cambridge: cambridge university press. ochs, e., (1982). talking to children in western samoa. language in societg, 11, 77-104. ochs, e. (1986). introduction. in b. schieffelin, & e. ochs, (eds)., language sodalization across cultures. cambridge: cambridge university press. ochs, e. (1988). culture and language development: language acquisition and language socialization in a samoan village. cambridge: cambridge university press. ochs, e. & schieffelin, b. (1984). language acquisition and socialization: three developmental stories and their implications. in r. shweder, & r. levine, (eds)., culture theorg: essags on mind, self, and emotion. cambridge: cambridge university press. philips, s. (1983). the invisible culture: communication in classroom and communitg on the warm springs indian reservation. new york: longman. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykins, vol. 39,1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 anne van kleeck ramey, c. & ourth, l. (1971). delayed reinforcement of vocalization rates in infants. child development, 42, 291-29 7. rees, n. & gerber, s. (1992). ethnography and communication: social-role relations. topics in language disorders, 12 (3), 15-27. rice, m. (1986). mismatched premises of the communicative competence model and language intervention. in r. schiefelbusch, (ed)., language competence: assessment and intervention. san diego: college-hill press. ryan, j. (1974). early language development: towards a communicational analysis. in m. richards, (ed)., the integration of a child into a social world. cambridge: cambridge university press. sachs, j. (1983). talking about the there and then: the emergence of displaced reference in parent-child discourse. in k. nelson, (ed)., children's language, vol. 4. hillsdale, nj: lawrence erlbaum. saville-troike, m. (1982). the ethnography of communication: an introduction. baltimore: university park press. schaffer, h., collis, g. & parsons, g. (1977). vocal interchange and visual regard in verbal and pre-verbal children. in h. schaffer, (ed)., studies in mother-infant interaction. london: academic press. schegloff, e. (1972). notes on a conversational practice: formulating place. in d. sudnow, (ed)., studies in social interaction. new york: free press. schieffelin, b. (1979). getting it together: an ethnographic approach to the study of the development of communicative competence. in e. ochs, & g. schieffelin, (eds)., developmental pragmatics. new york: academic press. schieffelin, b. & eisenberg, a. (1984). cultural variation in children's conversations. in r. schiefelbusch, & j. pickar, (eds). the acquisition of communicative competence. baltimore: university park press. schieffelin, b. & ochs, e. (1986). language socialization. annual renew of anthropology, 15, 163-191. scollon, r. & scollon, s. (1981). narrative, literacy, and face in interethnic communication. norwood, nj: ablex. shewan, c. (1988). omnibus survey: adaptation and progress in times of change. asha, 30 (8), 27-30. shorter, j. (1978). the cultural context of communication studies. theoretical and methodological issues. in a. lock, (ed)., action, gesture, and symbol. london: academic press. snow, c. (1977). the development of conversation between mothers and babies .journal of child language, 4, 1-22. snow, c. (19 79). the role of social interaction in language acquisition. in a. collins, (ed)., children's language and communication: 12th minnesota symposium on child psychology. hillsdale, nj: lawrence erlbaum. snow, c. (1986). conversations with children. in p. fletcher, & m. garman, (eds)., language acquisition (2nd edition). cambridge: cambridge university press. snow, c. (1989). imitativeness: a trait or a skill. in g. speidel, & k. nelson, (eds)., the many faces of imitation in language learning. new york: springer-verlag. stella-prorok, e. (1983). mother-child language in the natural environment. in k. nelson, (ed)., children's language, vol. 4. new yorkgardner press. tharp, r.jordan, c., speidel, g., hu-pei au, k„ klein, t., calkins, r., sloat, k., & gallimore, r. (1984). product and process in applied developmental research: education and the children of a minority. in m. lamb, a. brown, & b. rogoff, (eds)., advances in developmental psychology (vol. 3). hillsdale, nj: lawrence erlbaum. todd, g. & palmer, β. (1968). social reinforcement of infant babbling. child development, 39, 591-596. van kleeck, α., & carpenter, r. (1980). the effects of children's language comprehension level on adults' child directed talk. journal of speech and hearing research, 23, 546-569. vogt, l.jordan, c. & tharp, r. (1987). explaining school failure, producing school success: two cases. anthropology and education quarterly, 18, 276-286. von sturmer, d. (1980). rights in nurturing: the social relations of childbearing and rearing amongst the kugu-nganychara. master's thesis, australian national university, canberrra. ward, μ. (19 71). them children: a study in language learning. prospect heights, illinois: waveland press. watson-gegeo, k. & gegeo, d. (1986a). calling-out and repeating routines in kwara'ae children's language socialization. in b. schieffelin, & e. ochs, (eds)., language socialization across cultures. cambridge: cambridge university press. watson-gegeo, k. & gegeo, d. (1986b). the social world of kwara'ae children: acquisition of language and values. in j. cook-gumperz, w. corsaro, & j. streeck, (eds)., children's worlds and children's language. berlin: mouton de gruyter. wells, g. (1982). influences of the home on language development. in a. davies, {ha)., language and learning at school and home. london: heinemann. werner, e. (1984). child care: kith, kin and hired hands. baltimore: university park press. westbv, c. (1990). ethnographic interviewing: asking the right questions to the right people in the right ways .journal of childhood communicative disorders, 13 (1), 101-111. westby, c. & rouse, g. (1985). culture in education and the instruction of language learning-disabled students. topics in language disorders, 5 (4), 15-28. williamson, s. (1979). tamil baby talk: a cross-cultural study. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of pennsylvania. wills, d. (1977). culture's cradle: social, structural, and interactional aspects of senegalese socialization. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of texas at austin. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 49 geskrewe en gesproke afrikaanse sintaktiese vermoens van hardhorende kinders enid slabber, b(log] (pretoria] rene hugo, d.phil (pretoria) brenda louw, d.phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria opsomming die doel van hierdie ondersoek is om 'n moontlike verband tussen ekspressiewe geskrewe en gesproke sintaktiese vermoens van gehoorgestremdes te ondersoek. vyf afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremde proefpersone is gebruik. in die evaluering van sintaktiese vermoens is daar gekyk na die gebruik van 7 sinstrukture asook na woordvolgorde. uit die resultate blyk dit dat meer komplekse sinstrukture tydens die proefpersone se gesproke as geskrewe taal voorgekom het. die persentasie korrekte sinstrukture wat gebruik is, is egter groter in geskrewe as in gesproke taal, alhoewel die persentasie nogtans betreklik laag is. meer woordvolgordefoute is tydens geskrewe as gesproke taal gemaak. in die gebruik van die geevalueerde sinstrukture het daar egter meer woordvolgordefoute in gesproke as geskrewe taal voorgekom. die gevolgtrekking is bereik dat daar wel 'n verband tussen die gehoorgestremde proefpersone se geskrewe en gesproke sintaktiese taalvermoens is, maar heelwat verskille het ook voorgekom. navorsing op hierdie gebied le egter nog braak, en die huidige studie kan as vertrekpunt dien vir verdere navorsing. abstract' the aim of this research report is to examine the relationship between the expressive written and spoken syntactic abilities of hearingimpaired children. 5 afrikaans speaking hearing-impaired subjects were used. in the evaluation of syntactic abilities, 7 syntactical structures as well as word order, were evaluated. from the results obtained, it appeared that more complex sentence structures were used in spoken than in written language. the percentage structures used correctly, was higher in written than in spoken language, although the percentage was relatively low. more errors of word order occurred in written than in spoken language, but subjects evidenced more word order errors in spoken than in written language, when using more complex sentences. the conclusion is made that a relationship between written and spoken syntactic abilities of hearing-impaired subjects does exist, but differences were abo evidenced. the study can be used as a "starting point" for further research. vir normale taalverwerwing 1. inleiding normale gehoor is essensieel en gevolglik vertoon gehoorgestremde kinders deurgaans taalprobleme (reseptief en ekspressief) wat op verskillende taalvlakke en in verskillende grade manifesteer en hulle kommunikasievermoens nadelig be'invloed (davis en hardick, 1981). wanneer daar na kommunikasie verwys word, sluit dit enige wyse in waardeur die persoon sy voorgenome boodskap oordra. die uitdrukking van 'n boodskap kan deur middel van gebare, gesproke of geskrewe taal plaasvind. ekspressiewe taal (hetsy geskrewe of oraal) is vir die gehoorgestremde kind 'n probleem wat oorkom moet word ten einde die beperking op kommunikasievermoens op te hef of te verminder. hierdie feit is algemeen bekend, maar wat die verband tussen die twee ekstreme vorme, naamlik geskrewe en gesproke taal is, en wat die implikasies daarvan vir die spraakterapeut/oudioloog in die evaluering en behandeling van die gehoorgestremde kind is, is vrae wat nog beantwoord moet word. navorsingsbelangstelling is gerig op sowel geskrewe en ''esproke taalvermoens van gehoorgestremdes, maar outeurs bestudeer geskrewe en gesproke taal gewoonlik apart. beperkte navorsing is uitgevoer in terme van 'n vergelyking van geskrewe en gesproke taalvermoens (arnold, crossley en exley, 1982; power en wilgus, 1983). beskikbare data dui egter dat daar op alle taalvhkke beide ooreenkomste en verskille tussen gesproke en geskrewe taalvaardighede van gehoorgestremde kinders gevind is (arnold, crossley, exley, 1982; power en wilgus, 1983; myklebust, 1965). die doel van hierdie artikel is om die ekspressiewe geskrewe en gesproke sintaktiese vaardighede van gehoorgestremdes te bestudeer. sodoende word gepoog om 'n moontlike verband tussen die twee modaliteite te bepaal, en om die verkree inligting as moontlike riglyn tydens die evaluering en behandeling van die sintaksis van gehoorgestremdes aan te wend. * voetnoot * die gebruik van gebaretaal as kommunikasiemiddel deur gehoorgestremde kinders word erken maar word nie in hierdie ondersoek in berekening gebring nie aangesien slegs die geskrewe en gesproke vorms van afrikaans as verskynsel by gehoorgestremde kinders ondersoek word. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 © sasha 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 enid slabber, rene hugo en brenda louw 2. metode 2.1 proefpersoonseleksie tydens die seleksie van proefpersone is die standerd 4 a en b en standerd 5 a en b klasse van 'n bepaalde skool vir hardhorendes gebruik. op grond van bepaalde kriteria is 5 proefpersone uit 'n aantal van 70 kinders geselekteer, deur die leers van die 70 leerlinge na te gaan. die onderwyseres, spraakterapeut en sielkundige se menings is ook ingewin, om sodoende geldigheid van seleksie te verseker. die relevante kenmerke van die 5 kinders wat uiteindelik geselekteer is, word in tabel 1 aangedui. tabel 1: beskrywing van die relevante eienskappe van die geselekteerde proefpersone kriteria vir seleksie proefpersoonnommer 1 2 3 4 5 skoolplasing sonitus taal afrikaans geslag manlik dra van gehoorapparaat saktipe graad van gehoorverlies (gemiddelde suiwertoondrempel) linkeroor (db) 95 95 95 100 75 regteroor (db) 80 90 95 80 100 intelligensie gemiddeld invloed van stimulasie dagskolier meervoudige gestremdheid geen kongenitale gehoorverlies ja normaalhorende ouers ja ouderdomme in jare en maande 12.8 13.1 12.0 12.8 12.0 2.2 materiaal 'n toets vir die evaluering van afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremde kinders se ekspressiewe geskrewe en gesproke taalvermoens is opgestel, aangesien geen bestaande toetse aan die gestelde vereistes voldoen het nie. 2.2.1 teoretiese basis waarop die toets berus die benadering tot taalontwikkeling en taalafwykings wat deur bloom en lahey (1978) voorgestel is, word aangehang. hiervolgens bestaan taal uit drie komponente, naamlik taalinhoud, taalvorm en taalgebruik. die komponente wat deur bloom en lahey (1978) gedefinieer word, bied 'n raamwerk vir die evaluering van taalontwikkeling, en kan sinvol in die samestelling van die toets gebruik word. taalgebruik is egter nie in hierdie ekspriment geevalueer nie, aangesien dit moeilik toetsbaar is in geskrewe taal. in die bree kan die basis waarop die toets berus skematies (sien figuur 1) voorgestel word. 2.2'.2 toets vir die evaluering van afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremde kinders se ekspressiewe geskrewe en gesproke vermoens. die toets vir die evaluering van afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremde kinders se ekspressiewe geskrewe en gesproke vermoens, het beide geskrewe en gesproke taalvorm (sintaksis, morfologie en fonetiek) en taalinhoud (produktiwiteit, woordsoorte, aard) geevalueer, asook aanvullende inligting oor taalvermoens verskaf. 'n volledige uiteensetting van die toets met al sy onderafdelings word in bylae a verskaf. hierdie artikel verskaf egter net 'n bespreking van die sintaktiese vermoens van die proefpersone aangesien quigley, montanelli en wilbur (1976) meen dat sintaktiese vermoens beskou kan word as 'n verteenwoordigende weerspieeling van die gehoorgestremde kindysej vlak van ekspressiewe taalfunksionering. in die evaluering van sintaktiese vermoens, is twee aspekte, naamlik sinstrukture en woordvolgorde bestudeer. a) sinstruktuur hierdie afdeling omsluit die gebruik van die ingebedde sin, newe en onderskikkende sin, vraagsin, ontkenningsin, passiewe sin, onvoltooide sin, korrekte tydvorm van die sin en die gebruik van die direkte rede. hier word dus gekyk na die verskillende sinstrukture wat die gehoorgestremde kind korrek kan gebruik (arnold 1978; bunch 1979; powers en wilgus, 1983; steinkamp en quigley, 1977; pressnell, 1973;': ivimey, 1976; wilbur, goodhart en montandon, ,1983). b) woordvolgorde met foutiewe woordvolgorde word enige omruilings van1 twee of meer woorde, of slegs een woord wat in 'n verkeer-' de posisie in die sin gebruik word, bedoel. woordvolgorde is belangrik, aangesien dit 'n invloed het op die sinstruktuur en sintaktiese korrektheid daarvan. aangesien gehoorgestremdes geneig is om probleme met woordvolgorde te he, is dit ook by hierdie studie ingesluit (myklebust, 1965). 2.2.3 ontlokkingsprosedure ,· ^ beide geskrewe en gesproke taalvaardighede is afsonderlik ontlok deur prentbeskrywingstake. duidelike mondelinge instruksies is aan die proefpersone verskaf om 'n storie oor die voorgehoue prente te skryf en verbaal te vertel. 2.2.4 analise-prosedure sintaksis is geevalueer deur die strukture in elke sin te the south african journal of communication disorders, vol.34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) geskrewe en gesproke afrikaanse sintaktiese vermoens van hardhorende kinders 51 beoordeel ten opsigte van die frekwensie van voorkoms, korrekte woordvolgorde, weglating, vervanging of byvoeging van woorde in spesifieke sinstrukture gebruik. woordvolgorde is kwantitatief sowel as kwalitatief geevalueer en volgordefoute word met die strukture in die res van die sin in verband gebring. sodoende kan 'n duideliker beeld verkry word van wanneer die woordvolgorde in 'n sin verkeerd is, byvoorbeeld altyd saam met die gebruik van 'n voegwoord of voorsetsel, ensovoorts. 2.3 prosedure — vyf proefpersone is geselekteer op grond van gestelde kriteria. — 'n voorstudie is met 'n enkele proefpersoon uitgevoer en aanpassings op grond van die resultate wat verkry is, is gemaak. — by die toepassing van die finale weergawe van die toets op vyf proefpersone is geskrewe taalvaardighede eerste geevalueer om proefpersone sodoende meer op hulle gemak te stel. — gesproke taalvaardigheid is een week later geevalueer, sodat die herhalingseffek uitgeskakel kon word. 3. resultate en bespreking die verkree resultate van die proefpersone se geskrewe en gesproke sintaktiese vermoens word vervolgens bespreek. 3.1 sinstrukture die gegewens in verband met die tipe sinstrukture wat deur die proefpersone tydens die gesproke en geskrewe taal gebruik is, word in tabelle 2, 3 en 4 weergegee. tabel 2 verskaf 'n uiteensetting van die geevalueerde sinstrukture wat deur die proefpersone tydens gesproke en geskrewe taal gebruik is. in tabel 3 word opsommende inligting'aangaande die geevalueerde sinstrukture vervat. tabel 4/dui die voorkoms van persentasie foutiewe sinsvorme tydens gesproke en geskrewe taal aan. tabel 2: geevalueerde sinstrukture wat deur die proefpersone tydens gesproke en geskrewe taal gebruik is j sinstrukture i geskrewe taal gesproke taal a β c a β c ingebedde sin 0 0 0 0 ' 0 0 onderskikkende sin 6,3 20 0 2 100 0 neweskikkende sin 16,5 84,6 0 12,7 77 7,6 vraagsin 1,3 0 0 3 ' 100 0 ontkenningsin 6,3 80 0 9,8 40 10 passiewe sin 1,3 0 100 0 ι 0 0 direkte rede 1,3 100 0 17,6 5,6 5,5 a — persentasie sinstrukture gebruik β — persentasie sinstrukture korrek gebruik c — persentasie foutiewe woordvolgorde uit tabel 2 word afgelei dat die sinstrukture wat die meeste voorgekom het tydens geskrewe taal, die neweskikkende tabel 3: opsommende inligting aangaande sinstrukture geskrewe taal gesproke taal totale aantal sinne gebruik 79 102 totale persentasie geevalueerde sinstrukture 32,9% 45,1% persentasie t-eenhede nie geevalueer 67,1% 54,9% persentasie korrekte sinstrukture 65,4% 43,5% persentasie woordvolgordefoute in die gebruik van geevalueerde sinstrukture 3,8% 6,5% tabel 4: voorkoms van persentasie foutiewe sinsvorme tydens gesproke en geskrewe taal foutiewe sinsvorme % voorkoms in geskrewe taal % voorkoms in gesproke taal onvoltooide sinne tyd-vorm foutief 3,8 1,3 2 0 (16,5%), onderskikkende (6,3%) en ontkenningsinne (6,3%) was. ingebedde sinne kom glad nie voor nie, terwyl direkte rede, vraagsinne en passiewe sinne slegs 'n voorkomsfrekwensie van 1,3% getoon het. die sinstrukture wat die grootste persentasie korrekte voorkoms getoon het, was direkte rede (100% korrek), ontkenningsinne (80% korrek) en neweskikkende sinne (84,6% korrek). slegs 20% van die onderskikkende sinne is korrek gebruik. volgens tabel 3 bestaan 32,9% van die uitinge uit die sewe tipes sinstrukture wat in tabel 2 genoem word en daarvan is 65,4% korrek gebruik, terwyl slegs 3,8% van die bepaalde sinstrukture woordvolgordefoute getoon het. volgens tabel 2 is die sinstruktuur wat die hoogste voorkomsfrekwensie van die sewe tipes strukture getoon het tydens geskrewe taal neweskikkende sinne (16,5%). hierdie waarneming kan moontlik verklaar word deur die feit dat geen variasie in sinsvorm vereis word nie, en dit dus 'n minder komplekse sinstruktuur as die met 'n laer voorkomsfrekwensie is (heider en heider, 1940). die ingebedde, onderskikkende en passiewe sinne vereis egter 'n variasie in sinstruktuur, byvoorbeeld woordvolgorde, en dit is dus 'n meer komplekse sin (russel et al. 1976). powers en wilgus (1983) meen dat ingebedde, onderskikkende, passiewe, ontkenningsen vraagsinne almal 'n vorm van komplekse sinstrukture is, en die verkree resultate van hierdie studie stem dus ooreen met die van powers en wilgus (1983). 'n totale persentasie van 45,1 van alle sinne wat tydens geskrewe taal gebruik is, was van die sewe sintipes wat vir die ondersoek uitgesonder is. hiervan is 43,5% korrek gebruik. die hoe persentasie direkte-rede-sinne wat gebruik is, kan moontlik verklaar word op grond van die feit dat die direkte rede die taalvorm is wat elke dag tydens kommunikasie gebruik word. dit lyk asof die gehoorgestremde proefpersone in die direkte-rede-styl wat vir hulle bekend is, praat of 'n storie vertel. geen variasie in sinsvorm is nodig tydens direkte rede nie, wat ook moontlik verder bydra tot die hoe voorkoms hiervan. neweskikkende (12,7%) en ontkenningsinne (9,8%) kom daar waarskynlik met 'n hoe persentasie die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 voor omdat geen variasie in sinsvorm vereis word nie. neweskikkende sinne sluit ook die gebruik van die voegwoord "en" in, en volgens schwartz en solot (1980) is "en" een van die eerste en mees algemene voegwoorde wat deur die gehoorgestremde kind gebruik word. volgens schmitt (1970) is die gehoorgestremde kind nie geneig om passiewe sinne, ontkenningsinne of enige ander sinne wat variasie in vorm vereis, te gebruik nie. simmons (1962) meen ook dat die gehoorgestremde min buigbaarheid toon ten opsigte van sinstrukture, en die afwesigheid van sinne soos die passiewe vorm of ingebedde sin in die resultate kan moontlik hieraan toegeskryf word. die produksie van saamgestelde sinne is vir die gehoorgestremde kind moeilik en daarom word kort eenvoudige sinne gebruik, wat die klein persentasie (2%) onderskikkende sinne van die resultate verklaar (lewis en wilcox, 1978). pressnell (1973) is van mening dat die gehoorgestremde se bemeestering van sintaksis in gesproke taal vertraag is, en sy bevindinge word gestaaf deur huidige bevindinge. 'n vergelyking van die proefpersone se geskrewe en gesproke taal na aanleiding van die resultate wat in tabelle 2, 3 en 4 verskaf is, dui op die volgende ooreenkomste: — die resultate van beide die geskrewe en gesproke taal het getoon dat ingebedde sinne nie deur die proefpersone gebruik is nie. dit is aan die kompleksiteit van die sinstruktuur toegeskryf, asook aan die feit dat die sintaktiese reels vir hierdie struktuur nog nie bemeester is nie. tyack (1981) is van mening dat 'n kind met taalprobleme, probleme sal he met komplekse sinne, en dat die kind die reels van eenvoudige sinstrukture sal toepas wanneer hy met komplekse sinne in aanraking kom. die afleiding word dus gemaak dat gehoorgestremdes vertraagde taalverwerwing vertoon. — slegs 1,3% van die sinne in geskrewe taal was passiewe sinne en 0% tydens gesproke taal; dus 'n baie lae voorkomsfrekwensie van die passiewe struktuur. volgens power en quigley (1973) en wiig en semel (1980) toon die gehoorgestremde 'n groot agterstand met betrekking tot die verwerwing van die passiewe sinsvorm en die huidige resultate bevestig hierdie bevindinge. — neweskikkende (12,7% en 16,5% onderskeidelik) en ontkenningsinne (9,8% en 6,3% onderskeidelik) kom in sowel gesproke as geskrewe taal relatief baie voor in vergelyking met ander tipe sinstrukture, waarskynlik omdat dit nie sulke komplekse sinstrukture is nie en geen variasie in sinsvorm vereis word nie. heider en heider (1940) het bevind dat die gehoorgestremde tydens geskrewe taal 'n groter aantal eenvoudige sinne gebruik, (wat dus geen variasie in sinsvorm vereis nie), maar volgens die verkree resultate blyk dit die geval vir geskrewe sowel as gesproke taal te wees. di'e volgende verskille is tydens die vergelyking van die geskrewe en gesproke taal van die proefpersone opgemerk: — meer onderskikkende (6,3%) en neweskikkende (16,5%) sinne is in geskrewe as gesproke taal (2% en 12,7% onderskeidelik) gebruik. 'n moontlike verklaring hiervoor is dat meer tyd benut kan word tydens geskrewe taal om die sinstruktuur te beplan, terwyl gesproke taal vereis dat die spreker sy boodskap vinenid slabber, rene hugo en brenda louw nig moet beplan en verbaliseer. volgens northcott (1977) word die gehoorgestremde kind in gespreksituasies met kort, grammatikaal korrekte sinne deur sy ouers en onderwysers gestimuleer, wat dan moontlik aanleiding gee tot die gebruik van eenvoudige sinne in gesproke taal. tydens opleiding in die skool word daar heelwat aandag aan geskrewe taal gegee, wat dus ook 'n moontlike invloed op verkree toetsresultate kon he. — meer ontkenningsinne is in gesproke taal as in geskrewe taal gebruik (9,8% teenoor 6,3%), waarskynlik omdat alle kinders in die alledaagse lewe te doen kry met sinne wat ontkenning of negatiwiteit oordra (byvoorbeeld in die skool of tuis sal woorde soos nie, moenie en nee, dikwels gebruik word). hierdie ontkenningsinne word dan weer deur die gehoorgestremde kind tydens gesproke taal gebruik. hart en rosenstein (1966) is ook van mening dat die gehoorgestremde kind se taal sy linguistiese opvoeding wat hy van sy ouers en onderwysers ontvang, reflekteer. — meer vraagsinne (3%) en meer direkte rede (17,6%) is in gesproke taal as geskrewe taal (1,3% en 1,3% onderskeidelik) gebruik. in 'n skoolopset word die beantwoording van vrae dikwels van die gehoorgestremde kind vereis, en omdat vrae 'n persoon in staat stel om meer inligting van sy luisteraar te verkry, kan die hoe voorkomsfrekwensie van vraagsinne tydens gesproke taal moontlik met hierdie stelling verklaar word. die direkte rede is 'n taalstruktuur wat tydens alledaagse kommunikasie gebruik word, en dit wil dus voorkom asof die gehoorgestremde kind hierdie bekende struktuur ook in sy gesproke taal toepas; met ander woorde, direkte rede word nie herlei tot indirekte rede nie, aangesien die direkfe-rede-struktuur meer bekend is vir die gehoorgestremde kind. hierdie verklaring sluit dus weer eens aan by hart en rosenstein (1966) se bewering, soos vroeer vermeld. — tydens geskrewe taal is die direkte rede 100%'korrek gebruik, maar tydens gesproke taal is dit slegs in 5,6% van die gevalle korrek gebruik. hierdie diskrepansie kan moontlik toegeskryf word aan 'n tekort aan ouditiewe toevoer tydens gesproke taal, wat dan die lae persentasie korrekte gebruik van die direkte-redestruktuur verklaar. tydens kommunikasie deur middel van skryftaal is visuele toevoer egter die belangrikste sisteem wat as kontrolemeganisme dien, en dit bied 'n moontlike verklaring vir die 100% korrektheid wat voorkom (myklebust, 1964). 1 — tydens gesproke taal is onderskikkende en vraagsinne 100% korrek gebruik, terwyl slegs 2% en 0% onderskeidelik van hierdie sinstrukture tydens geskrewe taal korrek gebruik is. aangesien geskrewe taal eers vanaf 'n latere ouderdom aangeleer word, kan verkree resultate moontlik op grond hiervan verklaar word. / ' — tydens gesproke taal kom daar by 6,5% van die geevalueerde sinstrukture woordvolgordeprobleme voor, teenoor die 3,8% in geskrewe taal. volgens wiig en semel (1980) dui woordvolgordefoute op 'n subtiele prosesseringsprobleem by die gehoorgestremde se taal. schwartz en solot (1980) beweer dat die gehoorgestremde se woordvolgordegebruik beter in gesproke the south african journal of communication disorders, vol.34,1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) geskrewe en gesproke afrikaanse sintaktiese vermoens van hardhorende kinders 53 taal as in geskrewe taal sal wees, aangesien mislukkings voorkom kan word deur die gebruik van pouses. dit lyk egter asof die gehoorgestremde proefpersone in hierdie eksperiment nie in staat is tot die produksie van korrekte woordvolgorde nie, as gevolg van 'n onsekerheid omtrent korrekte woordvolgorde of 'n swak bemeestering van interne taalreels (schwartz en solot, 1980). — foutiewe sinsvorme (tabel 4) het telkens meer tydens geskrewe (3,8% en 1,3% onderskeidelik) as gesproke taal (2% en 0% onderskeidelik) voorgekom. die gehoorgestremde kind is nie daartoe in staat om die korrekte grammatika wat hy tydens gesproke taal aanwend in geskrewe taal toe te pas nie (bastable in arnold, crossley en exley, 1982). wanneer die resultate van geskrewe en gesproke taal samevattend beskou word volgens die inligting in tabelle 2, 3 en 4 vervat, word opgemerk dat die sewe sinstrukture wat geevalueer is, 32,9% van die sinstrukture wat tydens geskrewe taal gebruik is, uitmaak teenoor die 45,1% tydens gesproke taal. dit lyk dus asof daar meer komplekse sinstrukture in gesproke as in geskrewe taal voorkom. tydens geskrewe taal is 65,4% van die sinstrukture egter korrek gebruik, teenoor die 43,5% tydens gesproke taal, dit wil se meer van die komplekse sinstrukture word in gesproke taal gebruik, maar 'n groter aantal korrekte sinne kom tydens geskrewe taal voor. hierdie huidige resultate stem dus nie ooreen met die van arnold, crossley en exley (1982) se bevinding dat die gehoorgestremde meer foute tydens die skryf van sintaktiese eenhede maak as wanneer hulle praat nie. die verkree resultate stem egter ooreen met die van kretschmer en kretschmer (1978) wat noem dat die gehoorgestremde relatief min komplekse sinne in beide geskrewe en gesproke taal gebruik. resultate word verder ondersteun deur die bevindinge van navorsers soos ivimey (1976); wilbur, goodhart en montandon (1983) en bunch (1979). 3.2 woordvolgorde die tweede afdeling in die evaluering van sintaksis, is woordvolgorde. foute wat deur die proefpersone gemaak is met betrekking tot woordvolgorde, word in tabel 5 uiteengesit. ! tabel 5: resultate ten opsigte van proefpersone se woordvolgordefoute geskrewe taal gesproke taal % foute % foute % woordvolgordefoute gemaak in alle t-eenhede 5,1 4,0 % woordvolgordefoute gemaak tydens sinstrukture geevalueer (raadpleeg tabel 3) 3,8 6,5 volgens tabel 5 kom woordvolgordefoute by 5,1% van alle sinne in geskrewe taal voor, en by 3,8% van die sewe geskrewe sinstrukture s o o s igeevalueer (kyk ook tabel 4). twee verledetydsinne, asook by passiewe sinne. die woordsoort wat die grootste voorkoms van woordvolgordeprobleme veroorsaak, is die werkwoord. uit 'n kwalitatiewe analise blyk dit dat die voornaamwoord "wat" verwarring veroorsaak aangesien woordvolgordefoute ook in teenwoordigheid hiervan voorkom. tydens gesproke taal het die proefpersone by 4% van alle sinne woordvolgordefoute vertoon, en woordvolgordefoute by 6,5% van komplekse sinstrukture (tabel 5). kwalitatiewe analise van die resultate dui daarop dat woordvolgordefoute tydens die gebruik van die direkterede-struktuur, 'n neweskikkende sin, verledetydsvorm, en 'n ontkenningsin voorkom; dit wil se, meestal die sinne wat variasie in sinsvorm vereis en dus meer kompleks is. soos reeds vroeer genoem, het navorsing bewys dat die gehoorgestremde kind probleme met komplekse sinstrukture ondervind (heider en heider, 1940; simmons, 1962). uit tabel 5 blyk dit dus dat meer probleme met woordvolgorde tydens geskrewe taal (5,1%) as in gesproke taal (4%) voorkom. 'n moontlike verklaring hiervoor is dat geskrewe taal eers aangeleer word op 'n heelwat later stadium as die gesproke taal, en dus is die gehoorgestremde kind nog nie so vaardig in korrekte ordening van woorde as byvoorbeeld tydens gesproke taal wat elke dag vanaf geboorte gehoor word nie (alhoewel tot 'n beperkte mate by die gehoorgestremde kind). verskeie navorsers op die gebied van kindertaalontwikkeling huldig die mening dat die posisie van woorde in 'n sin belangrik is ten opsigte van betekenisvolheid en dat woordvolgorde reeds op 'n vroee stadium van taalontwikkeling 'n meganisme is wat betekenis uitdruk (sinclair en bronckart, 1972; brown, 1973; bloom, 1970). volgens brown en bellugi (1964) is dit moontlik dat die normaal horende kind normale woordvolgorde behou, omdat hy bepaalde semantiese verhoudings daardeur wil uitdruk. moontlik kan hierdie genoemde stelling dan ook dien as motivering waarom minder woordvolgordeprobleme tydens gesproke taal deur die proefpersone gemaak is as. tydens geskrewe taal. tydens die gebruik van meer komplekse sinstrukture (tabel 2), is daar egter 6,5% woordvolgordefoute in gesproke taal; teenoor die 3,8% tydens geskrewe taal gemaak. die moontlikheid is reeds genoem dat die gehoorgestremde kind tydens geskrewe taal meer tyd gegun word om die sinstruktuur te beplan as tydens gesproke taal, en daarom die kleiner persentasie woordvolgordefoute in komplekse geskrewe sinstrukture. celliers (1981) is van mening dat die kommunikasiewaarde van woordvolgorde vir normaal horende kinders op verskillende taalontwikkelingstadiums verskil, en aangesien die gehoorgestremde meestal 'n agterstand toon in die verwerwing van taal (presnell, 1973), kan ons verwag dat hierdie aspek nie vergelykbaar sal wees by die twee groepe nie. pressnell (1973) beweer dat die gehoorgestremde kind se bemeestering van sintaksis in gesproke taal vertraag is, en dit word dan bevestig deur die hoe persentasie woordvolgordefoute in die gebruik van komplekse sinstrukture. 'n verdere verklaring vir bogenoemde verskynsel mag wees dat die gehoorgestremde kind nie werklik oor die nodige begrip vir komplekse sinne beskik nie, en dus verward raak in die gebruik daarvan. dit is ook moontlik dat die toepaslike sintaktiese reels nog nie bemeester is nie. foute met woordvolgorde is gemaak tydens die gebruik van wanneer die navorsing (celliers, 1981) van normaal horendie suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 enid slabber, rene hugo en brenda louw des se woordvoigordegebruik met die van die gehoorgestremde kind vergelyk word, kan woordvolgordeprobleme aan die hand van die volgende verklaar word: — dit is moontlik dat die gehoorgestremde kinders nie die semantiese betekenis van uitinge volkome insien nie, en daarom is woordvolgorde nie vir hulle so belangrik nie. — 'n tweede moontlikheid is dat die gehoorgestremde kinders nie oor die sintaktiese vermoe beskik om te weet wat die korrekte woordvolgorde is nie. — dit blyk ook moontlik te wees dat die kommunikasiewaarde van woordvolgorde vir die gehoorgestremde kinders nie van soveel belang is nie. — laastens bestaan die moontlikheid dat die gehoorgestremde kinders 'n agterstand vertoon met betrekking tot die ontwikkelingsverloop van woordordereels, aangesien soveel foutiewe sinstrukture ook tydens hierdie eksperiment waargeneem is (celliers, 1981). 4. samevatting 'n vergelyking van die proefpersone se geskrewe en gesproke sintaktiese vermoens het op die volgende ooreenkomste gedui, naamlik: — die resultate van beide die geskrewe en gesproke taal het getoon dat ingebedde sinne nie deur die proefpersone gebruik is nie. — slegs 1,3% van die sinne in geskrewe taal was passiewe sinne en 0% tydens gesproke taal; dus 'n baie lae voorkomsfrekwensie van die passiewe sinstruktuur, wat ooreenstem met bevindinge van powers en quigley (1973). — neweskikkende (12,7% en 16,5% onderskeidelik) en ontkenningsinne (9,8% en 6,3% onderskeidelik) kom in sowel gesproke as geskrewe taal relatief baie voor in vergelyking met ander tipes sinstrukture. die volgende verskille is tydens die vergelyking van geskrewe en gesproke sintaktiese vermoens opgemerk, naamlik: — meer onderskikkende (6,3%) en neweskikkende (16,5%) sinne is in geskrewe as gesproke taal (2% en 12,7% onderskeidelik) gebruik. — meer ontkenningsinne is in gesproke taal as in geskrewe taal gebruik (9,8% teenoor 6,3%). — meer vraagsinne (3%) en baie meer direkte rede (17,6%) is in gesproke taal as geskrewe taal (1,3% en 1,3% onderskeidelik) gebruik. — tydens geskrewe taal is die direkte rede 100% korrek gebruik, maar tydens gesproke taal is dit slegs 5,6% van die gevalle korrek gebruik. i — tydens gesproke taal is onderskikkende en vraagsinne 100% korrek gebruik, terwyl slegs 2% en 0% onderskeidelik van hierdie sinstrukture tydens geskrewe taal korrek gebruik is. — tydens gesproke taal kom daar by 6,5% van die geevalueerde sinstrukture woordvolgordeprobleme voor, teenoor die 3,8% in geskrewe taal. die persentasie woordvolgordefoute gemaak in alle t-eenhede is egter 5,1% in geskrewe taal, teenoor die 4,0% in gesproke taal. op grond van die resultate van hierdie ondersoek as geheel kan die volgende gevolgtrekkings gemaak word, naamlik: — meer komplekse sinstrukture het tydens die proefpersone se gesproke taal voorgekom as wat in geskrewe taal gebruik is. — die persentasie korrekte sinstrukture wat gebruik is, is egter groter in geskrewe taal as in gesproke taal, alhoewel nogtans betreklik laag. — meer woordvolgordefoute is tydens geskrewe as gesproke taal gemaak. — die diskrepansie wat tussen die resultate van gesproke en geskrewe taal voorkom, mag 'n direkte gevolg van die onderrigwyses van die proefpersone wees waar gesproke taal as kommunikasiemedium aangeleer word en geskrewe taal bloot as 'n meganiese funksie aangeleer word. kretschmer en kretschmer (1978) se bevinding dat die gehoorgestremde kind min komplekse sinstrukture in gesproke en geskrewe taal gebruik, ondersteun dus die resultate van hierdie studie. aangesien dit blyk asof die gehoorgestremde proefpersone 'n onvermoe ten opsigte van die gebruik van komplekse sinstrukture toon, word aanbeveel dat meer blootstelling van die gehoorgestremde kinders hieraan nodig is. aangesien gehoorgestremde kinders in 'n voorgeskrewe skoolprogram opgeneem is en blootstelling in 'n groot mate bepaal word deur die kurrikulum wat gevolg word, is dit 'n belangrike faktor wat in aanmerking geneem moet word. die gebruik van komplekse sinstrukture en die korrektheid van woordvolgorde in die taal van gehoorgestremdes is egter 'n navorsingsgebied wat verdere indringende aandag moet geniet. / j 5. slot i ι die inligting wat tydens hierdie studie verkry is, hou belangrike implikasies in vir die evaluering en behandeling van die gehoorgestremde kind, deurdat meer insig omtrent die gehoorgestremde se gesproke en geskrewe sintaktiese vermoens verkry is. navorsing op hierdie gebied le egter nog braak, en die huidige studie is die eerste poging in afrikaans om die verband tussen geskrewe en gesproke sintaktiese vermoens uit te lig. waardevolle riglyne vir die evaluering en behandeling van gehoorgestremde 'kinders is egter vekry. die studie kan dus as vertrekpunt dien vir verdere navorsing. verwysings arnold, p. 1978. the deaf child's written english can we measure its quality? journal british association of teachers of the deaf, 2 (6), 196-199. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol.34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) geskrewe en gesproke afrikaanse sintaktiese vermoens van hardhorende kinders 55 arnold, p., crossley, e. and exley, s. 1982. deaf children's speaking, writing, comprehension of sentences. the journal of auditory research, 22, 225-232. bloom, l. 1970. language development : form and function in emerging grammars, cambridge, mass: mit press. bloom, l. and lahey, m. 1978. language development and language disorders, new york: john wiley and sons, inc. brown, r. 1973. a first language: the early stages, cambridge: harvard university press. brown, r. and bellugi, u. 1964. three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. harvard educational review, 3, 133-151. bunch, g.o. 1979. degree and manner of acquisition of written english language rules by the deaf. american annals of the deaf, 124 (1), 10-15. celliers, a.s. 1981. die ontwikkeling van woordordereels in sekere sintaktiese strukture by die jong afrikaanse kind. ongepubliseerde m-verhandeling, universiteit van pretoria. davis, j.m. and hardick, e.j. 1981. rehabilitative audiology for children and adults, new york: john wiley and sons. hart, b. and rosenstein, j. 1966. examining the language behavior of deaf children. the volta review, 66, 679-683. heider, f. and heider, g.m. 1940. a comparison of the sentence structure of deaf and hearing children. physiological monographs, 52, 42-102. ivimey, g.p. 1976. the written syntax of an english deaf child: an exploration in method. british journal of disorders of communication, 11 (2), 103-120. kretschmer, r.r and kretschmer, l.w. 1978. language development and intervention with the hearing impaired. baltimore: university press. lewis, t. and wilcox, j. 1978. the perceptual use of semantic rules by normal hearing and hard-of-hearing children. journal of communication disorders, 11, 107-118. myklebust, h.r. 1964. the psychology of deafness, new york, grune and stratton. myklebust, h.r. 1965. development and disorders of written language. (volume 1) picture story language test, new york: grune and stratton. northcott, w.h. 1977. curriculum guide. hearing impaired-children and their parents, washington: alexander graham bell association for the deaf, inc. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 power, d.j. and quigley, s.p. 1973. deaf children's acquisition of the passive voice. journal of speech and hearing research, 16, 5-11. powers, a.r. and wilgus, s. 1983. linquistic complexity in the written language of hearing-impaired children. the volta review, 85 (4), 201-210. pressnell, l.m. 1973. hearing-impaired children's comprehension and production of syntax in oral languag e. journal of speech and hearing research, 16, 12-21. quigley, s.p., montanelli, d.s. and wilbur, r.b. 1976. some aspects of the verb system in the language of deaf students. journal of speech and hearing research, 19, 536-550. russel, w.k. quigley, s.p. and power, d.j. 1976. linguistics and deaf children, washington: alexander graham bell association for the deaf, inc. schmitt, p.j. 1970. deaf children's comprehension and production of sentence transformations and verb tenses. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university microfilms, ann arbor, michigan. schwartz, e.r. and solot, m. 1980. response patterns characteristic of verbal expressive disorders. language, speech and hearing services in schools. 11, 139-144. simmons, a.a. 1962. a comparison of the type token ratio of spoken and written language of deaf and hearing children. the volta review, 64, 417-421. sinclair, h. and bronckart, j.p. 1972. svo a linguistic universal? a study in development phsycholinguistics. journal of experimental child psychology, 14 (3), 329-348. steinkamp, m.w. and quigley, s.p. 1977. assessing deaf children's written language. the volta review, 8, 7-13. tyack, d.l. 1981. teaching complex sentences. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 12, 49-56. wiig, e.h. andsemel, e.m. 1980. language assessment and intervention for the learning disabled, columbus, ohio: charles e. merrill publishing company. wilbur, r., goodhart, w. and montandon, e. 1983. comprehension of nine syntactic structures by hearing-impaired students. the volta review, 85 (7|, 328-345. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 enid slabber, rene hugo en brenda louw bylae toets vir die evaluering van afrikaanssprekende gehoorgestremde kinders se ekspressiewe geskrewe en gesproke taalvermoens a. taalvorm 1. sintaksis a) sinstrukture 3. fonetiek sinstrukture geskrewe taal gesproke taal ingebedde sin aantal woordvolgorde foutief korrek onderskikkende sin aantal woordvolgorde foutief korrek neweskikkende sin aantal woordvolgorde foutief korrek raagsin aantal woordvolgorde foutief korrek ntkenningsin aantal woordvolgorde foutief korrek direkte-rede-sin aantal woordvolgorde foutief korrek passiewe sin aantal woordvolgorde foutief korrek taalkorrekte foutiewe tipe verlanggelewerposimedia woord woord fout de klank de klank sie in of skrifof skrifwoord simbool simbool geskrewe taal gesproke taal b. taalinhoud 1. produktiwiteit aspekte van taalinhoud geskrewe taal gesproke taal geevalueer gesproke taal a) totale aantal woorde b) totale aantal t-eenhede c) woorde per t-eenheid 2. woordsoorte geskrewe taal gesproke taal woordsoorte a β c d a β c d werkwoord hulpwerkwoord van tyd modale hulpwerkwoord koppelwerkwoord selfstandige naamwoord voegwoord voorsetsel voornaamwoord bywoord lidwoord a — aantal β — byvoeging c — vervanging d — weglating 3. aard van taalinhoud t-eenheid nommer 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. ^ foutiewe sinsvorme geskrewe taal gesproke taal 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. ^ onvoltooide sinne tyd — vorm foutief 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. ^ b) woordvolgorde 6c 7. 8. t-eenheid nommer t-eenheid nommer geskrewe taal gesproke taal 9. 10. 11. 9. 10. 11. 2. morfologie 12. 13. morfologiese verbuigings geskrewe taal gesproke taal 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. * aantal * byvoegings * vervangings * weglatings 14. 15. 16. 17. 18. 19. geskrewe taal gesproke taal the south african journal of communication disorders, vol.34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) geskrewe en gesproke afrikaanse sintaktiese vermoens van hardhorende kinders 57 20. 21. 22. 40. opsommend: aard van taalinhoud geskrewe taal gesproke taal 23. vlak 1 24. vlak 2 25. vlak 3 26. vlak 4 27. 2 8 · c. aanvullende inligting 29· a) behoud van tema 30· b) herhaling van vorige idees 31· c) selfkorreksie 32. d) gebruik van selfstandige naamwoorde in plaas van voor33. naamwoorde 34· e) gebruik van inleidende en slotsinne 35· f) vooronderstelling 36. 37. algemene inligting omtrent taalvorm 38 3 g' algemene inligting omtrent taalinhoud opmerkings needier westdene has been active in the south african hearing market for more than 20 years and therefore fully understands your specific requirements in the areas of: testing of hearing ability improvement of hearing ability prevention against industrial noise supplying specialised electromedical equipment and tools for / spe'ech therapists, ent's and others. maintenance ^ n d repair to and calibration of all equipment supplied by needier westdene (and certain other suppliers). contact us first for: — professional ladvice and guidance — competitive quotes for products and consumable items — the best preand post-sales support. the needler westdene organisation c.c. (ck87/02503/23) p.o. box 28975 sandringham 2131 r.s.a. tel: 011-640 5017 member/lid: dr. s.j. van tonder, phd., mbl. general manager/algemene bestuurder die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s designed and manufactured in south africa the phantom series combines the flexibility of behind-the-ear hearing aids with the acoustic advantages and cosmetic appeal of in-the-ear hearing aids. a unique construction technique allows various models to be tried and tested in an individually made shell. clip-in modules are inserted with no special tools in a few seconds and it is even possible to make acoustic modifications to the earmould for controlling in-situ performance — including "open" earmoulds. m a n u f a c t u r e d by: a c o u s t i m e d (pty) l t d 327 b o s m a n b u i l d i n g cor. eloff a n d bree streets j o h a n n e s b u r g tel: (011) 337-2977 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 3, 198 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die voorkomsfrekwensie van ooren gehoorpatologie by kinders in die afgesonderde gemeenskap van tshikunda — malema santie meyer m(log)(pret.) carlin van den berg b(log)(pret.) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming hierdie navorsing het ten doel die bepaling van die aard en voorkoms van ooren gehoorpatalogie by 'n afgesonderde gemeenskap in venda. honderd drie-en-vyftig kinders is aan 'n siftingsprosedure wat uit 'n oor-, neusen keelondersoek, impedans en suiwertoonsiftingsoudiometrie bestaan, onderwerp. uit die resultate blyk dit dat 15,2 % van die getoetste populasie ooren gehoorprobleme het. die patologie wissel van middeloorpatologie (13,4%) tot sensories-neurale patologie (1,8%). dit wil dus voorkom asof die voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme onder hierdie bevolking relatief laag is. dit bevestig vorige navorsing wat bevind dat die algemene gesondheid van hierdie bevolking goed is. abstract the aim of this research was to determine the incidence of ear and hearing pathology in an isolated community in venda. one hundred and fifty three children were subjected to a screening procedure that consisted of an ear, nose and throat examination, impedance and pure tone screening tests. the results suggested that 15,2% of the population had ear and/or hearing pathology. this pathology ranged from middle ear conditions (13,4%>) to sensory neural conditions (1,8%). from these results it appears that the incidence of ear and hearing problems in this population is relatively low. this confirms previous research that states the good health conditions of this community. die verskeidenheid van etniese groepe in suidelike afrika bied 'n unieke geleentheid vir die bestudering van siektetoestande byvoorbeeld ooren gehoorpatologiee wat geassosieer word met die lewenstyl en omgewing (van staden, 1983). in die verband is 'n stam van ongeveer 1 000 persone wat steeds die tradisionele lewenswyse van hul voorvaders behou, in die noordelike dele van die republiek van venda gei'dentifiseer, in 'n afgelee gebied wat as tshikunda-malema bekend staan (van staden, 1983)/ die algemene gesondheidstoestand van hierdie mense is uitsonderlik goed, veral as die afwesigheid van gereelde mediese dienste in ag geneem word (breighton 1983, van staden, nel en van zyl, 1982). die persone blyk ongeaffekteer te, wees deur siektes wat normaalweg geassosieer word met 'n gesofistikeerde lewenstyl en vesellose dieet, byvoorbeeld hoe bloeddruk en artritis (van staden, 1983, breighton, 1983). aangesien dit bekend is dat otitis media 'n hoe voorkoms toon in gebiede waar mediese sorg ontoereikend is en die populasie onbewus is van die implikasies van die toestand (northern en downs, 1984), ontstaan die vraag of dit ook die geval by hierdie vendastam 'n ondersoek na die voorkoms en aard van ooren gehoorpatologie is veral van belang as in gedagte gehou word dat selfs 'n geringe tydelike gehoorverlies, soms deur otitis media veroorsaak kan word. hierdie gehoorverlies kan 'n negatiewe effek op die ontwikkeling van kommunikasieen akademiesevermoens uitoefen (howie, 1977, downs, 1977), en dus 'n verminderde lewenskwaliteit tot gevolg he. verder kan die resultate van so 'n studie 'n belangrike bydrae lewer in die daarstelling van 'n basislyn van die voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme wat latere navorsers as verwysing kan gebruik (van staden, 1982). dit is veral ook van belang in die geval van tshikunda-malema se inwoners wat waarskynlik veranderinge in lewenstyl en eetgewoontes sal vertoon as gevolg van die moontlikheid van ontwikkeling van sekondere industriee in die toekoms van die gebied (breighton 1983). 'n moontlike uitvloeisel van die resultate is die inisiering van 'n gehoorkonserveringsprogram wat deur die lede van die gemeenskap self in stand gehou kan word. metode vir die doel van hierdie studie is kinders geselekteer wat in die tshikunda-malemagebied van venda woonagtig is. elke kind is aan 'n oor-, neusen keelondersoek sowel as 'n suiwertoonen impedanssiftingstoets onderwerp. deur hierdie drie prosedures te gebruik word effektiewe identifikasie van ooren gehoorprobleme verhoog (fisch, 1981). proefpersone kinders tussen die ouderdom van een en twaalf jaar is getoets omdat die grootste frekwensie van ooren gehoorpatologie voor die ouderdom van 12 jaar voorkom. kinders in hierdie ouderdomsgroep wat vrywillig by 'n gesondheidskliniek aangemeld het, is aan die siftingsprogram onderwerp. deur konkrete beloning is gepoog om soveel as moontlik kinders te motiveer om vir toetsing aan te meld. honderd drie-en-vyftig kinders is gevolglik getoets. in tabel 1 word die proefpersone in terme van geslag en ouderdom voorgestel. apparaat die apparaat wat vir die oor-, neusen keelondersoek gebruik is, is standaardapparaat, byvoorbeeld 'n koplig, neusspekulum en otoskoop. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 santi meyer en carlin van den berg tabel 1 voorstelling van proefpersone ouderdom manlik vroulik totaal 1 1 6 7 2 2 1 3 3 2 6 8 4 4 6 10 5 9 5 14 6 8 10 18 7 11 6 17 8 7 9 16 9 3 13 16 10 10 8 18 11 9 9 18 12 5 3 8 totaal 71 82 153 die apparaat wat in die gehoorsiftingsprosedure gebruik is, is 'n madsen model tbn 60 suiwertoonoudiometer. die oudiometer is volgens iso 1964 standaarde geyk en daagliks tydens gebruik psigo-akoesties geyk. die impedansoudiometriese siftingsprosedures is deur middel van 'n damplex d175 impedansmeter uitgevoer. die elektriese voorsiening is van 'n kragopwekker verkry. vir pedo-oudiometriese toetsing van kinders wat nie deur middel van die suiwertoonsiftingsprosedure getoets kon word nie, is van 'n zenith neometer en geykte geraasmakers gebruik gemaak. toetsomgewing klankdigte fasiliteite was nie vir suiwertoonsiftingsoudiometrie beskikbaar nie. 'n vertrek, weg van die kliniek-aktiwiteite, is vir die doel geselekteer. die vertrek was bevredigend vir siftingsdoeleindes. toetsafhemers oudiometriese toetsing is deur 'n volledige oor-, neusen keelondersoek deur 'n oor-, neusen keelarts voorafgegaan. daarna is oudiometriesesiftingsprosedures, volgens 'n voorafbepaalde prosedure, deur twee ervare oudioloe uitgevoer. as gevolg van die taalprobleem is daar van 'n tolk gebruik gemaak vir die gee van instruksies en verkryging van die gevalsgeskiedenis. prosedure dataversameling elke proefpersoon wat vir toetsing aangemeld het, het die volgende prosedure deurloop: 'n tolk verkry inligting ten opsigte van 'n geskiedenis van ooren gehoorprobleme van die ouers. in die geval van die ouer kinders, is die inligting van die kind self, verkry. — hierna ondergaan die kind 'n oor-, neusen keelondersoek. oorwas word verwyder sodat impedansoudiometrie suksesvol uitgevoer kan word. die otoskopiese bevindinge word aangeteken. — die proefpersoon ondergaan dan 'n impedansoudiometriese ondersoek. hierdie toetsing sluit in timpanometrie, meting van statiese compliance en akoestiese reflekse. hierna volg die suiwertoonsiftingstoets. indien die kind te jonk is vir suksesvolle kondisionering vir die uitvoering van die suiwertoontoets, is van gedragsobservering met aanbieding van geraasmakers, gebruik gemaak. dataverwerking vir die verwerking van die rou data is die toetsen ondersoekgegewens wat by elke kind verkry is, afsonderlik in tabelvorm uiteengesit. die gehoor is as normaal beskou indien die proefpersoon response gelewer het by 500 hz, 1khz en 2khz by 20 db gp en 25 db gp by 4khz (northern en downs, 1984). die proefpersoon slaag dus die suiwertoonsiftingstoets by hierdie drempelwaardes. gedragsobservering daarenteen, was subjektief van aard. die baba moes bevredigende lokaliseringsresponse op stimuli lewer (northern en downs 1984). in die geval van impedansoudiometrie, slaag die proefpersoon die siftingstoets indien 'n tipe a-timpanogram en 'n refleks by 105 db gp of laer verkry word (harford et al. 1978). vir die klassifikasie van die resultate van proefpersone met sereuse otitis media, is van die prosedure van cantekin (1983) gebruik gemaak. akute otitis media en chroniese otitis media is deur die oor-, neusen keelarts gediagnoseer. resultate en bespreking van die 153 kinders (306 ore) wat getoets is, kon 15 kinders (30 ore) nie suksesvol deur middel van die suiwertoonen impedansoudiometriese prosedures getoets word nie, (tabel 2). 'n totaal van 138 proefpersone (276 ore) is dus suksesvol deur middel van albei toetsprosedures getoets. label 2 kinders wat nie suksesvol deur middel van die toetsprosedures getoets kon word nie ouderdom aantal nie getoets deur middel van ouderdom aantal impedans suiwertoon 1-3 jr 9 2 9 4-12jr 6 2 6 totaal 15 4 15 soos verwag kan word was meer kinders in die jonger ouderdomsgroep (nege kinders) onsuksesvol getoets, teenoor die viertot twaalfjariges waar ses nie suksesvol getoets kon word nie. dit is verder ook belangrik om op te merk dat geen een van die kinders deur middel van die suiwertoontoets toetsbaar was nie, maar dat 'n oordeel ten opsigte van middeloorfunksionering wel deur middel van die impedansresultate gemaak kon word. impedansoudiometriese resultate kan dus 'n bydrae lewer wanneer geen samewerking vir suiwertoontoetsprosedures verkry kan word nie. (northern 1978). uit die resultate blyk dit duidelik dat met toename in ouderdom daar 'n geleidelike styging in die persentasie van normale oudiometriese resultate is. daar is egter 'n enkele uitsondering naamlik die sewejariges, waar die resultate op 'n afname in normale ooren gehoorfunksionering dui. dit kan in verband gebring word met' harvey en wilmot (1969) se bevinding, naamlik dat daar 'n geringe toename van middeloortoestande tussen vieren negejariges voorkom. die hoer voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme by hierdie groep word verklaar na aanleiding van die boonste lugweginfeksies wat dikwels op die ouderdom voorkom. vanaf elfjarige ouderdom is daar opvallend minder afwykende resultate. dit stem ook ooreen met die resultate van vorige navorsers (weber, mcgovern en zink, 1967). voorkoms van afwykende suiwertoonen/of 1mpedanss1ft1ngsoudiometriese resultate' waar figuur 1 'n beeld gee van die voorkoms van resultate wat binne normale perke vir suiwertoonen impedanssiftingsproseduy the sourh african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pie voorkomsfrekwensie van ooren gehoorpatologie by die afgesonderde gemeenskap van tshikunda-malema 73 % 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 20 (d ε 3 ~ (λ ε 2 o u a ep = ·§ ϋ ο ελ ^ ιό ο α ft. ο 10 0 1 3 10 11 12 5 6 7 8 9 ouderdom in jare figuur 1 voorkoms van normale suiwertoonen impedansresultate res is, word die voorkoms van afwykende resultate in tabel 3 en 4 weergegee. in tabel 3 word die proefpersone met 'n sensoriesneurale betrokkenheid aangedui, terwyl tabel 4 die proefpersone met middeloorpatologie weergee. tabel 3 proefpersone wat suiwertoontoetsing faal, maar impedanstoetsing slaag (3 kinders) ouderdom van elke proefpersoon ore met afwykende suiwertoonresultate suiwertoongemiddeld in afwykende ore in db ouderdom van elke proefpersoon ore met afwykende suiwertoonresultate links regs 3 jaar 2 30 30 8 jaar 2 50 65 12 jaar / / / 1 i 25 (hoe frekwensie verlies) totaal % 5 1,8% tabel 4 die voorkoms van afwykende impedansoudiometriese resultate ouderdom van proefpersone in jare afwykende resultate ouderdom van proefpersone in jare faal st faal imp faal st slaag imp aantal ore getoets % 1-3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 2 2 3 7 9 6 7 6 2 2 5 2 2 2 4 2 5 7 14 6 9 8 2 0 18 16 26 32 32 32 32 36 36 16 22,2 12,5 19,2 21,9 43,8 18,8 28,1 22,2 5,5 0 totaal 42 15 57 276 20,7 uit tabel 3 is dit dus duidelik dat vyf ore die suiwertoonsiftingstoets gefaal het, maar die impedanssiftingstoets geslaag het. hierdie resultate impliseer normale middeloorfunksionering maar verminderde gehoorsensitiwiteit wat op 'n sensories-neurale gehoorverlies kan dui. die driejarige kind vertoon binouraal suiwertoondrempels van 30 db wat op 'n geringe gehoorverlies dui. hierdie kind was egter moeilik kondisioneerbaar en die moontlikheid dat hy wel oor normale gehoor beskik, is goed. die kind het moontlik nie op sy drempel gereageer nie, maar eers op 'n hoer vlak. die agtjarige kind vertoon 'n matige binourale gehoorverlies terwyl die twaalfjarige slegs 'n geringe monourale hoe frekwensie gehoorverlies vertoon. 'n gebrek aan bevredigende agtergrondsinligting maak die oorsake van hierdie gehoorverliese moeilik bepaalbaar. uit tabel 4 blyk dit duidelik dat 20,7% van die populasie een of ander vorm van middeloorpatologie vertoon, terwyl slegs 1,8% (tabel 3) 'n sensories neurale betrokkenheid vertoon. die hoer voorkoms van middeloorpatologie teenoor sensories-neurale gehoorverliese stem ooreen met vorige navorsing. hierdie navorsing dui daarop dat meer as sowat 70% van alle gehoorprobleme, as gevolg van middeloorpatologie, in die skoolgaande populasie voorkom (harvey en wilmot, 1969). dit is verder opvallend dat meer kinders (42) slegs uitvalle op die impedanstoetsresultate toon, teenoor kinders (15) wat uitval op beide suiwertoonen impedanstoetsresultate. hierdie bevinding bevestig die stelling van brooks (1980) dat 'n beduidende aantal kinders met middeloorpatologie se gehoor as normaal beskou sou word, indien impedansoudiometrie nie deel van die toetsbattery sou wees nie. vyftien ore vertoon egter uitvalle op beide suiwertoonen impedansoudiometriese resultate. hierdie resultate kan op 'n gevorderde middeloorpatologie of 'n gemengde gehoorverlies dui. tabel 5 geslag van kinders met afwykende oudiologiese resultate st suiwertoon imp = impedans seuns dogters aantal met afwykende toetsresultate 20 20 totale aantal kinders getoets 67 71 % 30 (29,9%) 28 (28,2%) tabel 6 die aard van ooren gehoorpatologie diagnose aantal uit 276 ore getoets persentasie middeloorpatologie: sereuse otitis media akute otitis media chroniese otitis media — perforasies — cholestiatoom subtotaal sensories-neurale patologie subtotaal 29 2 5 1 10,5 0,7 1,8 0,4 middeloorpatologie: sereuse otitis media akute otitis media chroniese otitis media — perforasies — cholestiatoom subtotaal sensories-neurale patologie subtotaal 37 13,4 middeloorpatologie: sereuse otitis media akute otitis media chroniese otitis media — perforasies — cholestiatoom subtotaal sensories-neurale patologie subtotaal 5 1,8 totaal 42 15,2 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 santi meyer en carlin van den berg uit tabel 5 blyk dit dat feitlik ewe veel seuns (29,9%) as dogters (28,2%) afwykende oudiologiese toetsresultate vertoon. hierdie resultate stem dus nie ooreen met die navorsing van klein (1978) wat bevind het dat spesifiek otitis media, betekenisvol meer by seuns as dogters voorkom nie. aard van die ooren gehoorprobleme alhoewel die inligting uit tabelle 2 en 3 aandui dat 62 ore as afwykend beskou word, dui tabel 6 daarop dat slegs 42 ore gehoorpatologie vertoon. dit kan verklaar word aan die hand van die wyse waarop die groep met sereuse otitis media geklassifiseer is, na aanleiding van die voorstel van cantekin (1983). daar is dus van beide die otoskopiese ondersoek en impedanstoetsresultate in tabel 6 gebruik gemaak. die resultate in tabel 3 is slegs op grond van die impedanstoetsresultate saamgestel. die beskikbare data is geklassifiseer deur die slaag /faal-kriteria van die impedans en otoskopiese ondersoeke met mekaar in verband te bring (cantekin 1983). sodoende kon sereuse otitis media ge'identifiseer word. suiwertoonen impedanstoetsresultate is saam met die otoskopiese ondersoek gebruik vir identifikasie van sensories-neurale gehoorverliese. in die geval van akute otitis media, perforasies en cholestiatome is daar hoofsaaklik op die otoskopiese ondersoek staatgemaak. uit tabel 6 blyk dit duidelik dat 15,2 % van die venda-kinders wat getoets is, oorof gehoorpatologie vertoon. middeloorpatologie kom voor by 13,4% van die getoetste kinders. hierdie persentasie is opvallend laag wanneer dit met die hoer voorkoms van middeloorpatologie onder ander etniese kindergroepe vergelyk word. aangehaal uit corth en harris (1984) is die voorkoms van middeloorpatologie onder getoetste populasies: alaska 27%, eskimo 15%, indo-chinese vlugtelingkinders 68,8%. die voorkoms van slegs perforasies onder navajo-skoolkinders was 14%, eskimo-kinders 32%, alaskaanse kinders 17% en amerikaans-indiaanse kinders 6,5% (northern 1978, howie 1977, johnson en watrous 1978). slegs 1,8% van die getoetste vendakinders toon perforasies. die voorkoms van sereuse otitis media by die venda-kinders (10,5%) is ook heelwat laer as die 20,5% van die amerikaanse indiane (johnson en watrous 1978). dit is verder nodig om in gedagte te hou dat hierdie studie tydens venda se wintermaande uitgevoer is. die moontlikheid bestaan dus dat die voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme (15,2%) kan afneem indien die toetse in die somermaande herhaal word (klein 1978). opsommend wil dit dus voorkom asof die voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme by hierdie groep venda-kinders laag is. die stam kan dus nie as 'n hoe risiko-groep vir ooren gehoor-patologie beskou word nie. gevolgtrekking verskeie faktore wat bydraend is tot 'n hoe voorkoms van middeloorpatologie is al ge'identifiseer byvoorbeeld onder andere sosioekonomiese status, behuising, voeding, stedelike en landelike omgewing, klimaat, gebrek aan mediese dienste (klein 1978, corth en harris 1984). die verklaring van die hoe voorkoms van otitis media by die inheemse noord-amerikaanse bevolkingsgroepe word hoofsaaklik aan die lae sosio-ekonomiese status en faktore wat hiermee verband hou, toegeskryf (klein, 1978). in die geval van die venda-kinders is die sosio-ekonomiese status laag, (van nieuwenhuizen en oosthuizen, 1984), maar ten spyte daarvan is die vendas, soos reeds genoem, baie gesond (van staden, 1983). daar kan dus verwag word dat die voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme laag sal wees. dit is egter moontlik dat 'n lae sosio-ekonomiese status, soos deur 'n westerse industriele gemeenskap beoordeel, nie op die tradisionele lewenswyse van die venda van toepassing gemaak kan word nie. geld speel 'n relatief onbelangrike rol en hierdie mense is tot 'n groot mate selfonderhoudend, en afhanklik van die natuur vir hul daaglikse voedselbehoeftes. behuising in lae sosio-ekonomiese groepe is dikwels beknop en ontoereikend (klein, 1978). dit blyk egter nie die geval by die venda te wees nie. behuising is 'n hut wat van hout, klei en dekgras gebou is. die man woon in sy eie hut, terwyl die vrou en haar kinders (gemiddeld drie kinders) hul eie hut bewoon (van staden, nel en van zyl, 1982). voeding is 'n verdere faktor wat die voorkoms van middeloorprobleme verhoog. kinders wat geborsvoed word vertoon 'n laer insidensie van middeloorprobleme (klein 1978). volgens crous (1984) word die venda kind dikwels tot op twee jaar of selfs langer geborsvoed. hierdie faktor mag dus bydraend tot die relatief lae voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme wees. die feit dat die persone in 'n landelike omgewing woonagtig is, (klein, 1978) en die klimaat deur die jaar gunstig is, (temperatuur is nie laer as gemiddeld 15 °c nie) (van nieuwenhuizen en oosthuizen, 1984) dra waarskynlik by tot die venda se goeie algemene gesondheid, en dus die relatief lae voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme. · westerse mediese dienste, bjyk onvoldoende te wees aangesien die venda-gebied slegs drie tot vier keer per jaar deur mediese spanne besoek word. hierdie afgelee groep mense vertoon nogtans goeie algemene gesondheid en die voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme is waarskynlik om die rede ook laer as wat verwag sou word, 'n hoer voorkoms is verwag, as navorsing wat met inheemse amerikaanse rassegroepe gedoen is, in ag geneem word (johnson en watrous 1978). | i die lewenstyl en eetgewoontes van die inwoners van tshikiindamalema sal waarskynlik in die toekoms, as gevolg van westerse invloede, verander. dit is van belang dat die invloed van die' verandering op die aard en voorkoms van ooren gehoorpatologie, met die oog op 'n gehoorkonserveringsprogram gemonitor wiord. aanbevelings vir verdere navorsing 'n vergelykende studie waar die voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme van die landelike venda-kind met die voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme onder die stedelike swart kind vergelyk word. bepaling van voorkoms van ooren gehoorprobleme onder die volwassenes van tshikunda-malema. die voorkoms van ouderdomsdoofheid kan bepaal word.. bedanking die skrywers bedank die hans snijkers instituut vir die geleentheid wat daargestel is om hierdie navorsing te kon doen. the souh african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die voorkomsfrekwensie van ooren gehoorpatologie by die afgesonderde gemeenskap van tshikunda-malema 75 verwysings breighton, s. third world rheumatology. in carson dick, w. en moll, j.m.h. (eds) recent advances in rheumatology nr (3). edinburg; churchill-livingstone, 1983. brooks, d.n. impedance screening. in jerger, j., northern, j.l. clinical impedance audiometry (tweede uitgawe). massachusetts: american electromedics corporation, 1980. cantekin, e.i. algorithm for diagnosis of otitis media with effusion. in stool, s.e. en bluestone, c.d. (eds) studies in otitis media pittsburgh otitis media research centre, progress report. 92, 6, 1983. corth, s.b. en harris, r.w. incidence of middle ear disease in indochinese refugee school children. audiology, 23 27-37 1984. crous, j.m. lektrise, departement huishoudkunde en dieetkunde. persoonlike mededeling, 1984. downs, m. the expanding imperatives of early identification. in bess, f.h. childhood deafness: causation, assessment and management. new york: grune & stratton, 1977. fisch, l. development of school screening audiometry brit. j. audiology, 15, 87-95, 1981. harford, e., fox, j., clemis, j. impedance audiometry for identification of conductive component in school children. in harford, e.r., bess, f.h., bluestone, c.d., klein, j.o. (eds) impedance screening for middle ear disease in children. new york: grune & stratton, 1978. harvey, r.m. en wilmot, t.j. the incidence of deafness in childhood. j. laryngology otology, 83, 449-456, 1969. howie, v.m. acute and recurrent acute otitis media. in jaffe, b.f. (ed) hearing loss in children. baltimore: university park press 1977. johnson, j.s., watrous, b.s. an acoustic impedance screening program with an american indian population. in harford, e.r., bess, f.h., bluestone, c.d., klein, j.o. (eds), impedance screening for middle ear disease in children. new york: grune & stratton, 1978. klein, j.o. epidemiology of otitis media. in harford, e.r., bess, f.h., bluestone, c.d., klein, j.o. (eds) impedance screening for middle ear disease in children. new york: grune & stratton, 1978. northern, j.l. impedance screening in special populations state of the art. in harford, e.r., bess, f.h., bluestone, c.d., klein, j.o. (eds). impedance screening for middle ear disease in children. new york: grune & stratton, 1978. northern, j. en downs, m. hearing in children (derde uitgawe) baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1984. van nieuwenhuizen, e.f.j, en oosthuizen, j.s. sosio-ekonomiese opname in tshikunda-malema (venda) 'n tussentydse verslag. universiteit van pretoria: instituut vir bedryfs en ontwikkelingsosiologie. departement sosiologie, 1984. van staden, d.a. kennismaking met tshikunda-malema geneeskunde, 1, 37-39, 1982. van staden, d.a. health and disease in a traditional-living tribe in southern africa. ecology of disease, 2, 149-150, 1983. van staden, d.a., nel, w„ van zyl, m.l. groep a b-hemolitiese streptokokke in 'n tradisionele swart gemeenskap. s a med j., 62, 569-570, 1982. weber, h.j., mcgovern, f.j., zink, d. an evaluation of 1 000 children with hearing loss. j. speech hear. disord 32 343-354, 1967. ' ' d l e suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 37 knowledge and attitudes of a group of south african mine workers towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices elise kahan and eleanor ross department speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract the study aimed to explore the knowledge and attitudes of a group of mine workers regarding noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices. these aims were investigated via a questionnaire administered in a group setting to 55 underground mine workers. the main finding that emerged from the study was that respondents were poorly informed regarding the fact that noise was a health hazard. furthermore, the knowledge that respondents did possess, appeared to have been derived from the personal experience of working in noisy environments for many years, rather than from educational input. contrary to expectations, respondents did not view deafness as a status symbol but rather as a negative attribute. consequently, they were motivated to protect themselves from hearing impairment and to be educated about hearing and the effects of noise. the mine workers complained about discomfort when wearing hearing protective devices as well as feelings of insecurity due to inhibition of communication and the inability to hear warning signals. for these reasons, noise protection was mainly worn in situations perceived as noisy by workers themselves. these results are discussed in terms of their implications for the clinical practice of audiology; hearing conservation in the mining industry; further research; and the training and education of speech-language pathologists and audiologists. opsomming daar is met hierdie studie gepoog om die kennis en standpunte van 'n groep mynwerkers aangaande gehoorverlies, veroorsaak deur geraas, en die gebruik van gehoorbeskermende apparate te ontgin. die doelwit is ondersoek deur middel van 'n vraelys wat 'aan 'n groep van 55 ondergrondse mynwerkers uitgedeel is. die vernaamste bevinding wat deur die studie opgelewer is, was dat proefpersone nie voldoende ingelig is aangaande die feit dat geraas 'n gesondheidsrisiko inhou nie. verder isj die kennis waaroor proefpersone beskik het blykbaar verkry uit persoonlike ondervinding van jarelange werk in geraasomgewings eerder as deur middel van opvoedkundige insette. in teenstelling met verwagtinge het proefpersone doofheid nie as statussimbool beskou nie, maar veel meer as 'n negatiewe eienskap. hulle is gevolglik gemotiveer om hulself te beskerm teen gehoorbeskadiging en om ingelig te word betreffende gehoor en die gevolge van geraas. die mynwerkers het gekla oor ongerief wanneer hulle gehoorbeskermende apparate moes dra en ook oor gevoelens van onsekerheid asgevolg'van inhibisie van kommunikasie en die onvermoe om waarskuwingseine te hoor. om hierdie redes is geraasbeskerming hoofsaaklik gebruik in situasies wat deur die werkers self as raserig beskou is. hierdie resultate is bespreek in die lig van implikasies vir die kliniese praktyk van oudiologie; gehoorbeskerming in die mynnywerheid; verdere navorsing; en die opleiding en opvoeding van spraaktaalterapeute en oudioloe. hearing loss due to occupational noise exposure has been estimated to be the most prevalent industrial disease (sataloff & sataloff, 1987). the physiological and psychological effects of noise on humans have been recognised for centuries (miller & silverman, 1984). in fact, the first known reference to noise induced hearing loss (nihl) was found in the medical literature of the sixteenth century (howse, 1987). in addition, a steady increase in the intensity and prevalence of noise in both living and working conditions has been noted since the industrial revolution (sataloff & sataloff, 1987). moreover, as modern technology has become more sophisticated, greater recognition of the effects of noise on hearing has occurred. "noise adversely affects the quality of life and strong links exist between noise levels and accident rates. recognition of this causal relationship has prompted legislation which, in turn, has been instrumental in the design and institution of hearing conservation programmes" (kielblock, 1986, p.2). legislative acts which have either directly or indirectly affected industrial hearing conservation programmes include the factories, machinery and building work act (1941), the machinery and occupational safety act (1983) and the mines and works act (1956) which was amended in 1964 and 1989 and amalgamated in 1991 to form the minerals act no. 50 of 1991. acdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 elise kahan & eleanor ross cording to a notice in the star newspaper, may 1994, a commission of inquiry into safety and health in the mining industry was being planned. the purpose of this inquiry was to investigate all aspects of the legal regulation of health and safety in the mining industry as defined in the minerals act, no. 50 of 1991; and to make recommendations to the state president on improvements to the existing regulations. this commission of inquiry formed part of the national health plan for south africa (1994) formulated by the african national congress (anc). against this historical and legislative backdrop, noise has been defined as "any unwanted auditory, electric or other signal, a signal that interferes with the detection and discrimination of sound and sound having aperiodic waveform" (sheeley, 1985, p. 1079). "noise is capable of producing a hearing loss in two ways: the hearing loss could result from acoustic trauma (e.g., an explosion), or it could be produced from chronic exposure to noise" (miller & silverman, 1984, p.101). noise induced hearing loss typically begins in the high frequencies (especially 4000 hz) and progresses to involve speech frequencies with continuing exposure (jackler & kaplan, 1994). effects of noise on hearing may be divided generally into three categories: temporary threshold shift, permanent threshold shift and acoustic trauma (miller & silverman, 1984; guignard, 1973 in melnick, 1985). temporary threshold shift (tts) refers to a short-term elevation in the threshold of hearing which recovers gradually following the noise exposure. permanent threshold shifts (ptss) are those hearing changes which persist. ptss which result from acoustic trauma or a single encounter with very destructive noise do exist but are relatively uncommon. more frequently, hearing loss produced by the effects of noise exposure is a result of accumulation of noise exposure which is repeated on a daily basis over a period of years. studies conducted by the chamber of mines research organisation (comro) clearly show that noise induced hearing loss occurs frequently in numerous instances due to workers' excessive exposure to noise. these studies were first started by comro in 1974 and the first industry wide studies followed in 1979 (leger, 1985). according to south african labour statistics, at the end of 1993 there were 676,380 people employed in the mining and quarrying industry. it is difficult to calculate the number of these workers exposed to noise. it has, however, been pointed out by the national institute of occupational safety and health (niosh) that the number of workers exposed to noise in the mining industry far exceeds the number exposed to noise in the majority of other industries (kielblock, van rensburg, frans & marx, 1991). for this reason, formal hearing conservation programmes have been introduced in the mining industry. the fundamental objective of any hearing conservation programme (hcp) is to protect workers from suffering permanent hearing impairment as a result of excessive exposure to noise at their place of work (miller & silverman, 1984; kielblock, 1986; sataloff & sataloff 1987). personal protection by means of hearing protective devices (ear-muffs or insertable plugs) is the most common form of hearing conservation in the south african mining industry to date (kielblock, 1986; shearer, 1992; kielblock & van rensburg, 1988). in order for hearing conservation programmes to be effective, it is therefore imperative that those at risk wear appropriate hearing protection correctly and consistently. as early as 1961 maas (cited in newby & popelka, 1985) reported on a study by an insurance company in wisconsin, which found that only 22.5% of the 1,148 plants polled, indicated success in encouraging workers to accept ear protectors for a period of six months or more. reasons for not utilizing ear protection included discomfort, headaches, interference with hearing, and getting used to noise so that it was not regarded as bothersome. by the next decade, the situation did not seem to have improved greatly as mass (1975) revealed a failure rate of more than 50% of hearing conservation programmes conducted in the united states. one of the primary reasons for this failure rate appeared to be the fact that workers did not perceive noise as a health hazard. another feature of the resistance to avoiding noise exposure stems from anecdotal evidence on the "macho" image which some men feel the need to display. wearing ear protectors is perceived as a sign of weakness (lipscomb, 1988). more recently, kielblock (personal communication, 1994) hypothesized that a possible reason for south african mine worker's reluctance to wear hearing protective devices might be related to the positive status of mine workers. as noise is part of the mine workers' job, he suggested that noise induced hearing loss represents an emblem of pride for the mine worker in that it provides proof that he has worked on the mines. stewart (1988) highlights a further factor which is likely to influence mine workers' attitudes towards the use of hearing protective devices . he explains that the sound levels of all sources of noise to which workers are exposed, fall well below the pain threshold which is about 140dba. furthermore, in view of the fact that typical noise induced hearing loss does not interfere with speech discrimination, at least not during the early stages, the noise hazard is usually not perceived as real. kielblock & van rensburg (1988) studied a group of 480 novices and returners to the south african mining industry in order to explore the relationship between workers' perceptions of noise as a health hazard and their willingness or reluctance to wear hearing protectors. they found that more than 80% of their respondents did not perceive noise as a 'problem' in their jobs, while the most experienced workers were capable of identifying the most noisy work categories. permanent hearing loss, as a sequel to prolonged exposure to noise, was regarded as a health hazard by only 16% of the respondents. the need for hearing protection in noise was recognised in 65% of cases, but 47% of respondents were averse to protecting their own hearing, since they felt that they would not be able to hear anything while wearing hearing protectors. the attitude of the mine workers towards noise as a health hazard thus appeared to contribute to the high failure rate of the hearing conservation programmes. kielblock & van rensburg (1988) concluded that invariably the value of education (as a means of influencing attitudes) is under-estimated. they emphasize that the challenge to education is to convince employees that noise which is not painful can still lead to hearing loss which is not apparent at first. in other words, employees should be educated to regard noise as a hazard similar to more obvious hazards such as heat, the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) k n o w l e d g e and attitudes of a group of south african mine workers towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices electricity and falls of ground. despite the important findings yielded by this study, it must be borne in mind that the researchers asked r e s p o n d e n t s only three questions in order to minimise disruptions to work routine and productivity on the mine. the present research project endeavoured to extend and build on kielblock & van rensburg's pioneering research efforts by surveying a broader range of knowledge and attitudes held by mine workers, towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices. the opportunity to conduct the study, was provided by a diamond mine situated in the northern transvaal.' according to the 1993 annual report, the mine in question retained a five star safety grading in 1993, but safety performance deteriorated. the progressive disabling-injury incidence rate rose from 0,32 per 200,000 man hours worked in 1992, to 1,33 per 200,000 man hours worked in 1993. a revised safety management programme was formulated and is currently being implemented on the mine in order to reverse this adverse trend. the loss-control co-ordinator in charge of implementing the programme expressed the view that the research project would fulfil a vital need by providing important information which could hopefully be embedded in the revised safety management programme. furthermore, before the mine management embarked on a time-consuming and costly educational programme with their large workforce, it seemed advisable to first obtain more facts on workers' attitudes. it was hoped that a survey of mine workers' knowledge and attitudes towards noise-induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices, would not only improve existing guidelines for hearing conservation programmes with this section of the population, but also contribute knowledge and understanding to a relatively neglected research area. moreover, the central focus of health policy in south africa is currently on primary health care as articulated in the african national congress's publication: a national health plan for south africa (1994). hearing conservation, as a form of preventive medicine, falls within the ambit of primary health care, thus underscoring the relevance and appropriateness of the present study to the south african context. moreover, the fact that a commission of inquiry into safety and health in the mining industry, was being held at the time the research project was conducted, further underlined the timeliness of the study. methodology aims of study 1. to investigate mine workers' knowledge of noise as a health hazard. 2. to explore the attitudes of these mine workers towards hearing loss; and , 3. to examine their attitudes towards wearing hearing protective devices. subjects criteria for selection: 1) subjects were required to be males between the ages of 24 and 45 years. this criterion was adopted be39 cause writers such as jerger & jerger (1981) maintain that auditory sensitivity of subjects in this age range is unlikely to have been affected by exposure to noise, use of ototoxic drugs or to degenerative effects associated with age. however, it is recognised that this assumption is open to question. 2) subjects were required to present with normal hearing. "according to the law employees working in noise levels above 85db must have hearing tests at least once per year" (noise: a safety steward's manual, 1991, p.49). the advantages of this procedure is that it allows the workers to be transferred to quieter jobs before their hearing is significantly affected. the disadvantage is that managers may transfer workers to jobs with lower remuneration or dismiss workers on the pretext that they must be repatriated for medical reasons. in view of these possible consequences of hearing impairment, persons who had sustained a hearing loss were excluded from the study as it was felt that this factor might have influenced their employment experiences and consequently biased their responses to the questionnaire. 3) the third subject selection criterion was literacy i.e., the ability to read and write english, so as to prevent incorrect information being collected due to a lack of understanding of the research instrument. 4) the final criterion was that all subjects were required to be working underground, so that they would be exposed to similar environmental conditions, altered by type of work only. subject sample after obtaining permission from the management of the mine as well as the co-operation of the national union of mine workers, 55 miners employed at the mine in question, were recruited for participation in the study. assistance was sought from the medical and loss control specialists employed at the mine to ensure that all subjects met the selection criteria. a non-probability, convenience sample was used. according to bless & achola (1990 p.75) this sampling procedure consists of taking all cases on hand until the sample reaches the desired size. for practical reasons as well as safety considerations, the subject sample comprised all workers who belonged to the same shift and worked on the same level underground on the day on which the study was conducted. the level chosen by the mine management accommodated 60 persons working in different categories e.g., drillers, operators, mechanics and helpers. of these 60 persons, 55 met the subject selection criteria. unfortunately, for reasons of confidentiality, it was not possible to obtain information from the mine regarding the levels of noise to which these different categories of workers were exposed. description of subjects the sample comprised 55 male mine workers. the ages ranged from 25 to 45 years with a mean age of 36.6 years. the home languages of the subjects were: sotho 47.3%, zulu 5.5%, pedi 14.5%, shangaan 1.8%, afrikaans 16.4%, english 1.8% and xhosa 10.9% with 1.8% unknown. extent of work experience ranged from 3 years to 28 years with a mean of 13 years and 8 months. the categories of jobs worked included: helpdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 elise kahan & eleanor ross ers 50.9%, machine drillers 7.3%, operators 12.7% and mechanics 24.1%. while 18.2% of the respondents mentioned that they had previously suffered from external or middle ear pathologies e.g., otitis media, the vast majority, namely 81.8%, had experienced no problems with their ears. all subjects had been audiometrically assessed as having normal hearing. research design in order to investigate the aims of the study, a survey research design was employed. in terms of the size of the mining population, its demographic spread and restrictions of time and resources, the use of a short questionnaire, personally administered, in a group setting to a sample of mine workers, was believed to be the most appropriate methodological tool for this study. description of the questionnaire a 6-page questionnaire comprising 27 questions was constructed which could be completed in 15-20 minutes. the instrument was drafted in english and a speaker of fanakalo was availaible for the benefit of respondents who came from diverse language backgrounds. the introductory letter provided assurance of confidentiality and anonymity and explained the purpose of the questionnaire. according to bailey (1987) these factors often determine whether or not respondents co-operate in filling in the questionnaire honestly and completely. included with the covering letter was an informed consent statement which complied with the guidelines laid down by the committee for research on human subjects, university of the witwatersrand. the questionnaire was made up of the following sections: a) demographic questions; and b) knowledge and attitudinal questions that examined: i) knowledge of noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices. ii) attitudes towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices; and iii) perceptions of the extent of service provided by the mines in the area of hearing conservation. these two sections are described separately as follows: demographic information the aim of the first eight questions was to obtain a socio-demographic profile of the respondents by soliciting biographical information on age, home language, type of work engaged in, and duration of work experience, as well as to identify anyone who had experienced ear or hearing problems in the past. questions which investigated knowledge of and attitudes towards noise induced hearing loss, hearing conservation and services the present study aimed to identify the knowledge and attitudes of mine workers towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices. in view of the fact that english was the second language of the respondents targeted in the study, the majority of questions were formulated in a closed-ended format to facilitate ease of completion. the rationale for inclusion of the various items in the questionnaire was based on guidelines laid down by the national union of mine workers, relevant legislation, as well as the literature on hearing conservation. according to "noise: the safety steward's manual" of the national union of mine workers (num) (1991), the minerals act states that if it is not possible to bring the noise levels down to below 85db, management is obliged to implement a hearing conservation programme. the first step involved in achieving this goal is the requirement that management signpost all noisy areas. question 1 therefore endeavoured to ascertain whether respondents had previously observed these noise warning signs. questions 2, 3 and 4 investigated workers' knowledge and perceptions of noise in their working environments and its effects. these questions related to findings in the literature that workers apparently lack information and are uneducated about noise and its effects on hearing (kielblock & van rensburg, 1988; leger, 1985; shearer, 1992; noise: the safety steward's manual, 1991). management is further obliged to carry out hearing tests on every worker annually. if the noise level exceeds 105db, hearing tests must be conducted every six months. questions 5 and 6 therefore aimed at obtaining information on the frequency of hearing tests conducted on respondents. the publication "noise: the safety steward's manual" (1991, p.30) states that "workers don't know the results of their hearing tests." hence question 7 probed whether results of hearing tests had been explained to the mine workers. question 8 then explored whether respondents had been given information on the effects of noise on hearing. question 9 explored respondents' motivation to learn about noise and hearing, and question 10 investigated the manner in which respondents wished to obtain information on these topics. j question 11 aimed to investigate respondents' attitudes towards a hearing loss. kielblock (personal communication, 1994) suggested that one of the weaknesses of hearing conservation programmes was respondents' lack of education. he also hypothesised that they perceived a hearing loss as a sign that they were mine workers and therefore a symbol of pride. management, is expected, according to the sabs 0831983 code of practice for the measurement and assessment of occupational noise for hearing conservation purposes, to train workers on how to use hearing protection. hence question 12 inquired whether respondents wore any hearing protective devices. questions 13 and 14 focused on reasons for use or non-use of hearing protectors and question 16 attempted to ascertain whether training had been given in the use/ storage, cleaning and replacement of such devices. it is suggested in "noise: the safety steward's manual" (1991) that management give each worker the opportunity to try out a number of different types of hearing protection. question,15 therefore endeavoured to ascertain whether workers had in fact been given a choice of hearing protective devices. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) k n o w l e d g e and attitudes of a group of south african mine workers towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices questions 17 and 19 investigated respondents' attitudes towards hearing conservation programmes and were based on the hypothesis that if the mine workers viewed hearing loss as a positive attribute (kielblock, personal communication, 1994) or if they were uneducated about the connection between noise and hearing loss (kielblock & van rensburg, 1988; shearer, 1992; leger, 1985) then they would most likely view a hearing conservation programme as being unnecessary and hence not request that one be started on the mine. question 18 enquired if the mine already had a hearing conservation programme in place. the final item, that is, question 20, used an openended format which allowed respondents the opportunity to express any other views or comments on the topic under discussion. research protocol data analysis data were analysed using descriptive statistics derived from the sas computer package, and presented in the form of tables and bar charts. statistical procedures adopted were univariate analyses, which provided frequency distributions, means and ranges. cross-tabulations of certain variables were also carried out, as well as chi-square analyses to determine differences between sub-groups on certain items. open-ended questions were analysed qualitatively according to themes expressed by the respondents. results and discussion results of the study are presented according to the order in which items appeared in section β of the questionnaire1. submission of questionnaire to the human ethics research committee in accordance with the code of ethics for research on human subjects (university of the witwatersrand), the questionnaire was submitted to the relevant university committee to ensure that informants' physical, social and psychological welfare was protected and their dignity and privacy respected. pilot study the adequacy of the research instrument and the practical possibilities of carrying out the project were assessed in a pilot study. the questionnaire was submitted to a member of the mine management as well as a num official for approval before the pilot study was carried out. four underground mine workers from the mine were the subjects in the pilot study. these men were excluded as subjects in the field study. although an official from the mine had informed the researcher that all the mine workers were able to read and write english, the pilot study revealed that different workers demonstratejd different levels of understanding. it was therefore decided to utilize the services of a fanakalo interpreter employed at the mine in order to enhance understanding of the questionnaire by respondents. j j the field study x the study was conducted in a group setting underground in the workshop area. the researcher was present to answer any queries and two speakers of fanakalo were available if subjects needed clarification or translation of any questions or words. both translators had been briefed previously on the need to adopt a neutral, objective stance and to keep as close as possible to the original phrasing of questions so as to avoid biasing respondents' answers. singleton, straits & straits (1993, p.262) state that "interviewers must be trained to be sensitive to the way in which they may wittingly or unwittingly affect their interviewees' responses". recognition of the noise warning sign the mine workers' knowledge was explored by eliciting information on the percentage of respondents who recognised the noise warning sign, as well as the proportion who understood what the sign meant. the vast majority of respondents i.e., 53 (96.4%) of the 55 persons in the sample recognised the sign, while a similar proportion i.e., 52 (94.5%) understood the meaning of the sign. these results indicated that the notices at the shaft head, advising persons to wear hearing protectors when entering a noise zone, as well as the appropriate noise warning symbols, prominently displayed at all entrances and exits of noise zones i.e., areas where noise exposure was greater than 85dbhl, were effective in that the mine workers had seen them and understood why they had been erected. the implication of these findings is that the mine was carrying out the guidelines on noise warning signs laid down by the sabs and that these notices were being effectively seen and understood. respondents who felt they worked in noisy conditions the majority of respondents i.e., 42 (76.4%) felt they worked in a noisy place. a survey conducted by the chamber of mines suggests that the majority of underground day shift workers are exposed to an equivalent noise level of loodba or more (van rensburg, schutte, strydom, jooste & schoeman, 1980). the results obtained from the present study appeared to validate the statement made by the chamber of mines. according to sabs 083-1970, the hearing of 120 out of every 1000 workers will be impaired within five years if exposed to noise levels of this intensity. these figures imply that unless the hearing conservation programmes are successful or there is greater noise control engineering, there is likely to be a large number of workers presenting with hearing losses and an extensive hearing impairment liability for mine management in the form of compensation claims. 1 copies of the questionnaire are available from the second author, dept. of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, p.o. wits, south africa, 2050. die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. , 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 elise kahan & eleanor ross respondents who felt that working in noise affected hearing a total of 52 (94.5%) of the respondents felt that working in noise affected hearing. these results illustrate that the workers were aware of the dangers of noise. however, on further questioning to ascertain why the workers thought that noise affected hearing, many miners stated that they did not know the reason but realised that noise influenced hearing due to personal experience. some of the statements furnished by respondents which highlighted this point included the following: "because i can feel it afterwards." "the noise hurts my ears and i can't hear my friends on the way back from work." "self-experience." another common response was that when working in noise, the noise blocked out all other sounds including the sounds of people talking. these findings suggested that much of the workers' knowledge appeared to be derived from the personal experience of working in a noisy environment. this is understandable if one considers that the mean number of years worked on the mine, was 13 years 8 months. the majority of these workers felt that they worked in a noisy environment and sabs 083-1970 states that noise levels of 100 dba can cause 120 out of every 1000 workers to become hearing impaired within five years. the emphasis on prevention in the new government health care policy (a national health plan for south africa, 1994, p. 19) would appear to underscore the need for workers to be educated about the dangers of noise rather than learn about noise through physically experiencing its effects. in this respect professionals who have been trained in the field of audiology could fulfil a vital educational role by supplementing the valuable services currently provided by personnel from other disciplines involved with hearing conservation on the mines. respondents'perceptions of changes in their hearing status since working on the mine when respondents were asked about the stability of their hearing, 34 (61.8%) stated that they felt their hearing had remained the same, while 21 (38.2%) perceived their hearing to have deteriorated. this finding suggests that in 38.2% of the cases the hearing protection was either not effective or not being used. information on table 1. respondents' perceptions of their hearing status since working on the mines. cross tabulated with the use of hearing protective devices, while working perceived hearing status was cross-tabulated with data regarding the use of hearing protective devices while working, and the results depicted in table 1. the findings show that 22 (40%) of the respondents who wore their hearing devices compared with the 12 (21.9%) who did not wear these devices while working perceived their hearing to have remained the same, while 19 (34.5%) of respondents who wore their hearing protective devices and the 2 (3.6%) who did not use these devices perceived their hearing to have deteriorated. the findings imply that although 40% of respondents were wearing their hearing protection and finding their hearing to be the same, 34.5% stated that they were wearing their hearing protection and the hearing protection devices were not efficacious, i.e., conserving their hearing. a chisquared analysis indicated significant differences between these sub-groups (x21=4,543; ρ < 0.05). however, the data only represent workers' perceptions of their hearing status. in order to obtain more scientific data, one would have to correlate respondents' perceptions with audiometric test results. regularity of hearing tests all the respondents reported that their hearing was tested annually, prior to going on vacation. this finding demonstrates that the regulations laid down in the sabs 083-1983 were being effectively implemented at the mine in question. explanation of results table 2 shows the percentage of mine workers who stated that the results of their hearing tests had been explained to them. the majority of workers namely, 38 (69.1%) reported that they had not had the results explained to them. these data were cross-tabulated with the workers' home language. it is of interest to note that the majority of respondents who had had the results of their hearing test explained to them were afrikaans first language speakers. this finding may indicate that due to language barriers the results of the hearing tests are table 2. respondents who had/did not have their hearing test results explained to them, cross tabulated with their first language | yes no remained the same 40,0% 12,9% deteriorated 34,5% 3,6% total 74,5% 16,5% the south yes no unknown sotho 5,5% 40,1% 1,8% pedi 3,6% 10,9% 0 xhosa 1,8% 9,1% 0 ' afrikaans 12,8% 3,6% 0 english 1,8% 0 shangaan · 0 1,8%·' 0 zulu 1,8% x%% r 1,8% unknown 0 ,· 1,8% 0 total % 27,3% 69,1% 3,6% / journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) knowledge and attitudes of a group of south african mine workers towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices not explained to all workers. one possible way of dealing with language barriers could be for translators to be available to explain the results of the hearing tests to the workers. indeed, the effective use of a system of interpreters and translators could play a vital role in ensuring the success of hearing conservation programmes. such a system could firstly, educate the workers about their individual hearing as well as about hearing in general; and secondly, inform workers, so as to empower them to make their own decisions about their hearing i.e., whether they needed to be transferred to a quieter job or whether they should choose a different type of hearing protection device. as kielblock (1986) states: "the success of any hearing conservation programme hinges primarily on education and personal relevance." the explanation of the workers' hearing test results could go a long way towards fulfilling both these criteria. 43 attitudes towards the view that deafness is a positive status symbol for mine workers fig 3 demonstrates that the majority of respondents either disagreed or strongly disagreed with the statement that deafness is a positive status symbol for mine workers. there was only 1 (1.9%) respondent who strongly agreed and 2 (3.7%) who responded neutrally. this finding implies that the majority of the mine workers in the sample did not feel that deafness was a positive status symbol. this result appears to refute the idea expressed by kielblock to the researcher personally, that part of the challenge to a hearing conservation programme was the positive attitude of mine workers towards a hearing loss. however, the present research respondents who had been educated about the effects of noise on hearing fig 1 indicates the percentage of respondents who had been advised about the effects of noise on hearing. a total of 31 (56.4%), reported being informed; however, 24 (43.6%) were not informed. this percentage suggests that many workers were not receiving sufficient education/ information. kielblock (1986) maintains that this lack of education is the reason why programmes fail. it would therefore appear to be vitally important to enhance the educational side of hearing conservation programmes. respondents who requested information about noise and hearing fig. 2 shows that a large number of respondents namely 34 (61.8%) indicated a desire to learn more about noise and hearing. this finding may be due to the results obtained from question 3 i.e., that respondents' knowledge about noise and hearing had been derived primarily through personal experience rather than educational input. it would therefore be interesting to pose this same question to novice mine workers with limited work experience and compare results with those obtained from miners with longer work histories. ι medium of information requested for learning about the effects of noise on hearing this item probed the manner in which respondents wished to be informed about the effects of noise on hearing. the majority of respondents indicated a preference for video, while the remaining respondents were equally divided between a personal talk or a group talk. these results provide important information on'how to supplement existing hearing conservation programmes with additional input that will reach the workers and, hopefully, make the programmes more effective. through the medium of film/video, group talks and personal talks, and with the help of professionals in the field of noise and hearing, it is possible to educate and motivate respondents so as to improve the chances of hearing conservation programmes being successfully implemented. a ί ζ m ο a. 50.4% 43.6% y e s n o figure 1. respondents who had or had not been told about the effects of noise on hearing (n=55) cd < 60% ο 6 1 . 8 % γ • 9 . 1 % y e s n o u n k n o w n figure 2. respondents who requested information about noise and hearing. (n=55) % m ο a. m s t r o n g l y a g r e e figure 3. attitudes towards the view that deafness is a positive status symbol for mine workers. (n=55) die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 elise kahan & eleanor ross project was conducted on a relatively small convenience sample which precludes generalisation of the results to the broader population of mine workers. it is therefore suggested that the research be replicated on a larger, more representative sample and results compared. use of hearing protective devices while working the majority of mine workers i.e., 41 (74.5%) stated that they used hearing protection while working. reasons for the use/non-use of hearing protection while working mine workers' attitudes were examined by eliciting explanations for wearing or not wearing their hearing protective devices. qualitative responses included the following: "to protect the ear; to prevent deafness." "only when they are compulsory, when they are not compulsory noise can't be bad for you." "only when in loud noise." "depends where one is working, the noise is not very bad everywhere." from the above statements it appears that the majority of respondents wore hearing protection in noise that they considered to be dangerous for them. however, one is not sure whether or not respondents' definitions of loud noise meant noise greater than 85 dba as recommended by the sabs. this implies that workers may have had hearing protection available to them but may still have been exposing themselves to harmful noise due to incorrect judgement of the noisy situation. the only way to prevent this phenomenon from occurring seems to be via education. it would seem that workers need to be informed about what noise level is harmful and given an example/model of harmful noise intensity which they can use to assess whether or not they are in a hazardous noise zone. reasons for not wearing hearing protective devices while working, included the following: "inhibits communication." "can't hear warning signals." "uncomfortable." "causes skin irritation." "machines only come sometimes, so not so noisy." these examples indicate that the majority of reasons for not wearing hearing protection centred firstly, around the lack of comfort and secondly, the inability to "hear the hanging talk". with regard to the first aspect, comfort is one of the issues constantly addressed by employers, employees and manufacturers of hearing protective devices. it is also indirectly addressed in the sabs 083-1983 code of practice which states that workers must be provided with a choice of hearing protective devices. with regard to the second aspect, jones (1994, p.29) explains that "hanging talk is referred to by mine workers as a sort of primordial whisper as tiny fractures spread rapidly through the ground. the only other natural alarm is the scuttling of rats as they abandon miners to their fate." similar concerns have been found among workers who have to wear hearing protection world-wide (sataloff & sataloff, 1987; miller & silverman, 1984; kielblock, 1986; leger, 1985). the view that hearing protective devices inhibit communication is challenged and defended in various studies. kryteri, 1946, in leger (1985) found slight improvements in signal and speech intelligibility from wearing hearing protection. schroder, van rensburg, schutte & strydom (1980), also endorse the above view. endruweit & hach (1977), state that hearing protection improves speech communication in noisy environments; however, adaption is necessary and varies from person to person. kielblock (1986) explains that with hearing protection one hears better, but differently. wilkins & martin (1982) challenge some of these assumptions by saying that wearing hearing protection did not have any major effect on recognition of artificial warning signals such as sirens and alarms in the sample that they studied; however, the perception of environmental warning sounds was reduced. abel, alberti & riko (1982), showed that hearing protection did not have any effect for individuals with normal hearing but when individuals suffered from hearing loss, hearing protective devices reduced speech intelligibility considerably. taking all these points into consideration, it would seem that hearing protective devices do serve a purpose. nevertheless, they are not without faults, even allowing for proper induction, instruction and adaption. furthermore, as kielblock (1986) points out, hearing protection devices should be seen as an interim measure only until more effective noise reduction strategies can be implemented. in other words, noise intensity should be reduced at the source via quieter machinery rather than minimising noise for the receiver via hearing protective devices. whether respondents were given a choice of hearing protective devices analysis of data revealed that 27 (50,9%) of respondents reported not being given a choice of hearing protective devices. the publication entitled: "noise: a safety stewards manual" (1991) states that management must give each worker a number of different types of hearing protection to try out. kielblock (1986) states that one of the reasons many hearing conservation proputting them on storing them cleaning them when to replace y e s n o figure 4. training received on use of hearing protective devices. (n=55) / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) knowledge and attitudes of a group of south african mine workers towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices 45 g r a m m e s fail is the lack of choice of hearing protective devices. it is therefore recommended that all workers be provided with a choice of hearing protective devices. training received in the use of hearing protective devices fig 4 shows that the majority of the respondents stated that they did not receive training in any of the four areas i.e., inserting and removing; storing; cleaning; and replacing hearing protective devices. some of the workers in the study claimed that one of the reasons for not using their hearing protection was due to skin irritation or sores allegedly caused by these devices. proper instruction in the above four areas, especially storing, cleaning and replacing may help in alleviating some of these complaints, thereby helping workers to protect their ears as well as preventing them from experiencing unnecessary irritation and pain. moreover, the generally hot, dusty conditions which prevail in many underground mining areas and which tend to cause mine workers to perspire profusely, further underscores the need for proper cleaning and storage of hearing protective devices. hence instruction in the use and care of hearing protectors could probably be more cost effective for the mine than treating ear infections and paying hearing compensation claims. this type of educational strategy could also improve the success rate of hearing conservation programmes. views regarding a hearing conservation programme the vast majority of the respondents i.e., 52 (94.5%) felt that a hearing conservation programme was necessary and requested that the mine should start one. these results imply that these mine workers were motivated and therefore, if a comprehensive, integrated hearing conservation programme was started and properly implemented, there appeared to be a good chance of success being achieved. the publication entitled, "guidelines for the implementation and control of a hearing conservation programme in the south african mining industry" (1988) spates that the success of many hearing conservation programmes hinges on the extent of voluntary participation which in turn, stems from personal conviction. the mine workers in this study displayed an attitude conducive to the successful implementation of a hearing conservation programme. many respondents i.e., 39 (70.9%) realised that although the mine had implemented legislative guidelines in terms of hearing conservation, the mine had nevertheless, not implemented a comprehensive hearing conservation programme as defined in the questionnaire i.e., a programme which teaches mine workers, union officials and management how to reduce noise levels, how to conserve or save workers' hearing and how to prevent hearing loss. for this reason many of the respondents i.e., 39 (70.9%) correctly stated that the mine did not have a hearing conservation programme. although the mine had installed noise warning signs, issued free hearing protective devices, provided hearing tests every year and covered the topic of hearing in some ten minute lectures, these factors do not constitute a comprehensive, integrated hearing conservation programme. views articulated by the majority of respondents also highlighted the mismatch between what providers and recipients of a hearing conservation programme perceive as an adequate programme. the respondents who stated that the mine did have a hearing protection programme incorrectly assumed that the safety measures which had been legislated,for, and which were being carried out, constituted a comprehensive hcp. additional comments the respondents were given the opportunity to express any further comments. this item was formulated as an open-ended question. the following are examples of some of the responses furnished: "less noise on the mine." "give feedback on results." "would like more information on hearing protective devices." "if my hearing is becoming worse, who do i complain to? will i lose my job?" "would like to see a hearing conservation programme implemented on the mine." "would like to know if the mine has a hearing conservation programme." "would like a hearing test to be done every 6 months and the doctor must look inside the ear to see that there is no irritation, dirt or sore." "there is a lot of noise in the working place like the machines and fans. the mine must try to reduce the noise." "would like more training about ears, noise and hearing." these verbatim responses indicated that the mine workers in the study appeared to be lacking knowledge and were motivated to obtain help, guidance and assistance. the reason for this was probably due to their experience of working in noisy conditions and experiencing the effects of noise. conclusions the main finding that emerged from the study was that, in terms of knowledge, respondents were poorly informed regarding the fact that noise was a health hazard. furthermore, respondents' knowledge appeared to have been derived from the personal experience of working in noisy environments for many years rather than from educational input. contrary to expectations expressed by kielblock (personal communication, 1994), the majority of respondents did not view deafness as a status symbol and therefore did not have a positive attitude towards a hearing loss. they were motivated to protect themselves against deafness and to be educated about hearing and the effects of noise. furthermore, the mine workers' demonstrated positive attitudes towards wearing hearing protective devices in situations which they themselves perceived as noisy. they also complained about lack of comfort when wearing the devices as well as feelings of insecurity due to the inhibition of communication and the inability to hear warning signals. these were the primary reasons furnished by workers for only wearing these devices in situations perceived as noisy by the miners themselves. it is important to note that this study was conducted on a relatively small, unrepresentative, non-probability, convenience sample, which precludes generalisation die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 elise kahan & eleanor ross across the mining industry as a whole. nevertheless, the findings from the study do have implications for clinical practice of audiologists; hearing conservation in the mining industry; further research; and the training and education of speech-language pathologists and audiologists. clinical practice of audiologists the study highlighted the need for audiologists to play a vital role in the area of education and worker empowerment to help ensure successful hearing conservation programmes. according to the national health plan (1994), health policy in south africa advocates a primary health care (phc) approach which incorporates health programmes and services that emphasize both prevention and rehabilitation, and identify high-risk and under-served occupational groups. the audiologist would appear to be ideally equipped with knowledge and understanding in the area of hearing and noise induced hearing loss, to ensure that health policy and related legislation benefit at risk groups such as the mining population. hearing conservation in the mining industry findings from the study clearly show that there is more to a successful hearing conservation programme than simply implementing the safety regulations for hearing supplied by the sabs. the implication is that although mines such as the one where the study was conducted, appear to be making a concerted effort to conserve hearing, there is a need to establish comprehensive hearing conservation programmes which incorporate all the factors mentioned in both the guidelines for the implementation and control of a hearing conservation programme in the south african mining industry (1988), as well as the publication entitled "noise: a safety steward's manual" (1991). in particular, it is recommended that: (1) greater use be made of audiologists in the education of mine workers regarding noise as a hearing hazard. (2) results of hearing tests be explained to miners. (3) workers be provided with a choice of hearing protective devices. (4) mine workers be given training in the insertion, removal, cleaning, storage and replacement of hearing protective devices. research results from the present study cannot be generalised due to the relatively small convenience sample used. it is therefore recommended that the study be replicated on a larger, more representative sample which would more realistically reflect the mining populations' knowledge and attitude towards hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices. other recommendations for future research include the following: similar questions as the ones posed in the present study questionnaire could be put to a group of novice mine workers to see if similar results are obtained. knowledge and attitudes towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protective devices could also be related to audiometric assessments of respondents. similar studies on the knowledge and attitudes of workers towards noise induced hearing loss and the use of hearing protection devices, could be carried out with other at risk occupational groups in commerce and industry e.g., airline pilots and construction workers. training and education of speech-language pathologists and audiologists at present the training curricula of speech-language pathologists and audiologists seem to focus largely on the theory underpinning noise induced hearing loss and the principles of hearing conservation. it is recommended that more opportunities be provided for students to acquire the practical experience of educating various occupational groups regarding the hazards of noise, and of actively implementing hearing conservation programmes in the community, particularly in high noise-risk industries. acknowledgements the authors wish to express their sincere appreciation to brad taurog for his assistance with creating the figure diagrams. references a national health plan for south africa. (1944). johannesburg, south africa: african national congress (anc). abel, s.m., alberti, rw. & riko, k. (1982). user fitting of hearing protectors: attenuation results. in alberti, rw. (ed.). personal hearing protection in industry. new york: raven press. bailey, k.d. (1987). methods of social research. new york: the free press. bless, c. & achola p. (1990). fundamentals of social research methods: an african perspective. lusaka: government printer. chamber of mines user guide. manual number 11 (1988) research organisation. johannesburg: chamber of mihes. code of ethics for research on human subjects, (undated). johannesburg, university of the witwatersrand. ' de beers (1993). annual report. south africa: colour press. endruweit, g. & hach, k. (1977). do hearing protection shells adapt to interference with communication under noisy conditions? sicherheitsingenieur, pp.22-25. i ginsberg, i .a. & white, i.p. (1985). otologic considerations in audiology. in katz, j. (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology. (pp. 15-38), baltimore: williams & wilkins.1 howse, d.a. (1987). the role of the social support afforded'by trade unions as moderator of the stress-strain relationship. unpublished masters dissertation, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. jackler, r.k. & kaplan, m.j. (1994). ear, nose and throat. in tierney, l. & mcphee, s.j. & papadakis, m.a. (eds.). current medical diagnosis and treatment, (pp. 175-205), usa: appleton and lange paramount publishing business and professional group. jerger. j. & jerger. s. (1981). auditory disorders: a manual for clinical evaluation. boston: little, brown and co. jones, b. (1994). new method will aid mine safety. -the sunday times. 24 april p.29. kielblock, a.j. (1994). personal communication. kielblock, a.j. (1986). hear.no evil: a critical review of industrial hearing conservation programmes with special reference to the south african mining industry. loss control survey, (5), 5-13. k i e l b l o c k , a.j. & van r e n s b u r g , a.j. ( 1 9 8 8 ) . the the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) k n o w l e d g e a n d a t t i t u d e s of a g r o u p of s o u t h african m i n e workers towards n o i s e i n d u c e d h e a r i n g l o s s a n d the u s e of h e a r i n g p r o t e c t i v e d e v i c e s 47 implementation and control of hearing conservation programmes with reference to the south african mining industry. mine safety and health proceedings. johannesburg: chamber of mines of south africa, pp. 129135. k i e l b l o c k , a.d., van rensburg, a.j., franz, r.m. & marx, h.e. (1991). a review of hearing conservation in the south african mining industry. mine safety and health congress. the carlton hotel, johannesburg. leger, j.p. (1985). towards safer underground gold mining. an investigation commissioned by the national union of mine workers. tag / sociology research post. department of sociology, u n i v e r s i t y of the w i t w a t e r s r a n d , johannesburg. lipscomb, d.m. (1988). hearing conservation in industry, schools and the military. london: taylor & francis. mass, r.b. (1975). occupation noise exposure and hearing conservation. in zenz, c. (ed.) occupational medicine: principles and practical applications, (pp.317-359), chicago: year book medical publishers. melnick, w. (1985). industrial hearing conservation. third edition. in katz, j. (ed.). handbook of clinical audiology. (pp. 721-741), baltimore: williams and wilkins. miller, m.h. & silverman, c.a. (1984). occupational hearing conservation. englewood cliffs: prentice hall. noise. a safety steward's manual. (1991). national union of mine workers. j o h a n n e s b u r g : learn and teach publications. newby, h.a. & popelka, g.r. (1985). audiology. 5th ed. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall inc. sataloff, r.t. & sataloff, j. (1987). occupational hearing loss. new york: marcel dekker, inc. schroder, h.h., van rensburg, g.a.j., schutte, p.c. & strydom, n.b. (1980). noise and hearing conservation in the gold mining industry. johannesburg: chamber of mines. shearer, s. (1992). the sound of silence. goldfields review 1991 1992. (pp.65-70), johannesburg: goldfields. sheeley, e.c. (1985). glossary. in katz, j. (ed.). handbook of clinical audiology. third edition. baltimore: williams & wilkins. singleton, r.a., straits, b.c. & s t r a i t s , m.m. (1993). approaches to social research. new york: oxford university press. south african labour statistics. (1993). pretoria: central statistical services. stewart, a.p. (1988). the comprehensive hearing conservation program. in lipscomb, d.m. (ed.). hearing conservation in industry, schools and the military, (pp. 203-230), london: taylor and francis. van rensburg, a.j., schutte, pc., strydom, n.b., jooste, p.l.& schoemann, j.j. (1980). the personal noise doses of different workers in the gold mining industry. johannesburg: chamber of mines. (unpublished paper). wilkins, p.a. & martin, a.m. (1982). the effects of hearing protection on the perception of warning sounds. in alberti, p.w. (ed.). personal hearing protection in industry. new york: raven press. a p p e n d i x acts and related guidelines sabs code of practice for the assessment of noise exposure during work for hearing conservation purposes. sabs 0831970, south african bureau of standards: pretoria. sabs code of practice for the measurement and assessment of occupational noise for hearing conservation purposes. sabs 083-1983 as amended 1986 and 1989, the council of the south african bureau of standards: pretoria. the factories, machinery and building work act (1941). pretoria: government printers. the machinery and occupational safety act (1983). pretoria: government printers. the mines and works act no. 27 (1956) (amended 1964 and 1989). pretoria: government printers. the minerals act no. 50 (1991). pretoria: government printers. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 myrtle l. aron, robyn e. lewis and judy l. willemse reagan, t.g. sign language diversity in south africa: a preliminary investigation, with implications for the education of the deaf. unpublished proposal for a research project, 1986. schlesinger, h.s. the acquisition of signed and spoken language. in deaf children: developmental perspectives. liben, l.s. (ed). academic press, inc., new york, 1978. stokoe, w.c. the study and use of sign language. sign language studies. 10, 1-36, 1976. stokoe, w.c. sign and culture, linstok press, maryland, 1978. stokoe, w.c. and kuschel, r. a. a field guide for sign language research, linstok press, maryland, 1979. van der merwe, a.j. deputy director — special education, department of education and training, pretoria, personal communication, march, 1986. viljoen, s.w. communication skills project — editorial, silent messenger, south african national council for the deaf, v 5 no 3, july/august, 1982. wilbur, r.b. the linguistics of manual language and manual systems. in communication assessment and intervention strategies. lloyd, l.l. (ed). university park press, baltimore, 1976. woodward, j. implications for sociolinguistic research among the deaf. sign language studies, 1, 1-17, 1972. an intonational analysis of deaf speech: a case study marguerite schneider, ma (applied linguistics) (reading, u.k.) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract the intonation of an english speaking congenitally deaf adult was analysed using the framework set out by crystal (1969) for normal spoken english. the analysis revealed a deviant and deficient intonation system. the main features of this system included, firstly, an excessive use of tone units resulting in unintentionally emphatic sounding speech; secondly, inaccurate and inconsistent use of both the grammatical and accentual junctions of tonicity; and thirdly, a deficient tone system with additional abnormal use of the tones which formed the subject's tonal repertoire. this paper highlights the need for both phonetic and phonological analyses in order to obtain a true indication of a speaker's performance. opsomm1ng die intonasie van 'n kongenitaal dowe engelssprekende volwassene is geanaliseer deur 'n raamwerk te gebruik wat deur crystal (1969) vir standaard engelse spreektaal opgestel is. die analise het 'n qfwykende en gebrekkige intonasiesisteem aan die lig gebring. die belangrikste kenmerke van hierdie sisteem is eerstens, 'n oormatige gebruik van tooneenhede wat spraak wat onopsetlik beklemtoon klink tot gevolg het; tweedens, onakkurate en inkonsekwente gebruik van beide die grammatika en aksentsfunksies van toon; en derdens, 'n gebrekkige toonstelsel met addisionele abnormale gebruik van die tone wat die proefpersoon se toonrepertoire gevorm het. die noodsaaklikheid van beide fonetiese en fonologiese analises vir die vasstelling van 'njuiste aanduiding van 'n spreker se prestasie word deur hierdie ondersoek sterk na vore gebring. introduction the congenital handicap of hearing impairment affects the normal development of intelligible speech (ling, 1976). intelligible speech is a function of both segmental and non-segmental aspects of phonology. in other words, in order to produce intelligible speech a person must have mastered the individual phonemes as well as the intonation system of his/her language. (in addition, intelligible speech requires the mastery of syntactic, semantic and pragmatic aspects of language, but discussion of these areas is beyond the scope of this paper.) normal hearing children develop basic intonational contrasts before they acquire the finer phonemic distinctions which are to be fitted into the intonation 'envelope' or pattern (bruner, 1975; crystal, 1979). phonemic errors and segmental phonological systems of hearingimpaired speakers have been thoroughly investigated in the literature (ling, 1976; stark, 1979; dodd, 1976). however, it is not only the phonemic problems which render the speech of the hearing-impaired largely unintelligible. abnormal prosody also plays a large role in this process (john and howarth, 1965; phillips et al. 1968; silverman and calvert, 1978; king and parker, 1980). until recently the prosodic features of deaf speech were described in a rather impressionistic manner using labels such as 'monotonous voice' and 'laboured speech' without much formal analysis to justify these. it is only recently, with the advent of © sasha 1986 visual display aids (eg. laryngoscope and visispeech as described by king, parker, spanner and wright, 1982) that researchers have been able to analyse and describe the prosodic features of deaf speech in an objective manner, as well as compare them to the norm (martony, 1968; nickerson and stevens, 1973; nickersonet al. 1976; phillips et al. 1968; king and parker, 1980). i i stark (1979) summarises the typical prosodic features found in the speech of the deaf. these include: 1 —errors in timing —excessive word and sentence duration —errors in rhythm —intonation problems such as too little pitch variation (monotonous voice) or excessive pitch variation. however, there is no indication as to whether these features are used to mark meaning contrasts. in other words, are these features used on a phonological level as well as on a phonetic one? if one of the aims of deaf education is to teach effective verbal communication, it is imperative that deaf speakers be taught to make meaning contrasts both by phonemic and intonational means. in order to develop appropriate teaching goals and methods we need to establish not only the phonemic systems used by deaf speakers but also their intonational systems or lack of them. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) international analysis of deaf speech: a case study 73 crystal (1969) has established a detailed and comprehensive account of normal prosodic systems and intonation in english. he defines prosodic features as those "vocal effects constituted by variations along the parameters of pitch, loudness, duration and silence" (crystal, 1969: 128). the primary prosodic features include pitch, loudness and duration, while the secondary ones include rhythmicality and pause, which both involve combinations of the three primary features. these features allow the following prosodic systems to be distinguished. tone, which involves pitch movement pitch range, which involves relative pitch levels tempo, which involves relative speed loudness, which deals with changes along a soft/loud continuum relative to the preceding utterance — rhythmicality, which allows for the description of speech along the rhythmic/arhythmic dimension — pause, which deals with the use of pause within the stream of speech. these systems function across both monosyllabic and polysyllabic stretches of speech (crystal, 1969: 140). crystal (1969) distinguishes two functions of intonation. the first is the ability to impart attitudinal information, such as a puzzled or questioning attitude with rising intonation, a neutral attitude with falling intonation, and an ironic or sarcastic attitude with a flat intonation. the second function of intonation is grammatical, such as in the placement of the tonic or nuclear stress on the adjectives in "a blue pen, not a red one" but on the nouns in "a blue car, not a blue pen". in these two examples the second stress in the utterance has its placement determined by the placement of the first stress. all grammatical functions of intonation will also entail an expression of attitude, whereas an attitudinal function of intonation will not necessarily involve a grammatical function. intonation involves three aspects or components, namely tone units, tone and tonicity. these three aspects are described below. a) tone units: these are the most easily identified and defined. crystal (1969) defines the tone unit as having one peak of prominence in the form of a nuclear pitch movement followed by a tone unit boundary which is indicated by two phonetic factors: there is a perceivable pitchjchange, the direction of which is determined by the nuclear tone, and ii) there are junctural features at the end of every tone unit, normally in the form of a very slight pause, often accompanied by variations, such as in length and aspiration used in the production of j the final phonemes. without a nucleus the tone unit is incomplete. on a semantic level the tone unit can be seen as corresponding to a 'sense group', on a syntactic level to a clause (usually), on a physiological level to a 'breath group' (crystal, 1969: 205). b) tone: this component of intonation describes the direction of pitch movement of the nucleus or nuclear syllable of the tone unit. three groups of tones are identified by crystal (1969): i) simple tones include falling, rising and level tones which are undirectional. ii) complex tones include all nuclei "where there is a change in direction of pitch movement within a syllable" (crystal 1969: 217). complex tones include fall-rise, rise-fall, rise-fall-rise and fall-rise-fall, the first two being the more common. the syllable with the first element of the tone is more prominent phonetically than the syllable with the second or third element. iii) compound tones are "combinations of two kinetic elements of different major phonetic types acting as a single tonal unit (crystal, 1969:218). the two main types are fall + rise and rise + fall, where the "two elements of a complex tone have in effect been separated to allow a larger stretch of utterance to fall under the semantic range of the nucleus" (crystal, 1969: 218). as for complex tones, the compound tones have one element which is more prominent phonetically than the other, this usually being the first. when used together with the pitch range system one finds tones being produced with a narrow, wide or normal pitch range resulting in different meanings. in his data on normal intonation of english, crystal (1969) found that simple falling tones were by far the most commonly occurring tone type. c) tonicity: this involves the placement of the nuclear tone within the tone unit. tonicity has two functions: the accentual and grammatical functions. the accentual function is governed by non-linguistic situational factors and is unpredictable from the grammar. it serves to highlight the most relevant part of the 1 sentence. the grammatical function is "largely or wholly predictable from the context immediately preceding the tonic word" (crystal, 1969: 263), as in alternative contrasts such as for the utterance "are you going by train or by car" where the tonic placement on any other words than train and car would, in most contexts, render the sentence ungrammatical. in english it is generally the case that the nucleus falls on the last lexical item in the tone unit. the aim of this paper is to analyse, using the framework described above, the intonation of a congenitally deaf adult in order to establish whether she is using intonation in a systematic manner or not. the second aim, arising out of the first, is to establish the nature of this system, if there is one. methodology 1. the subject (m): a) description μ is a 23 year old female whose mother-tongue is english. she has a profound, bilateral sensori-neural hearing loss of congenital origin with unknown cause. the pure tone average for the frequencies 500 hz, 1000 hz and 2000 hz is 98 db for both ears. μ has worn bilateral hearing aids since the age of two and a half. m's parents and siblings are all hearing. b) education: μ attended schools advocating an oral approach. she has a higher national certificate in maths, statistics and computing and is employed as a computer programmer. c) speech characteristics: m's speech is mostly intelligible. she has some consistent segmental errors mainly involving consonants. these include: — weak fricatives — unreleased final plosives — some nasalisation — intrusive schwa. it was decided to choose a subject who had reasonably good segmental ability so that the intonational factors could be more easily isolated. 2. data collection and analysis: about 20 minutes of informal conversation between μ and the writer was recorded on a cassette recorder. the tape was then transcribed orthographically. only m's utterances were analysed in terms of prosodic features. the following features were marked: — tone unit boundaries — stress — tonicity — pitch range — nuclear tones — pauses and rhythm. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 marguerite schneider appendix a gives a list of the symbols used in the transcription. counts were made of the occurrence of the various features and percentages were calculated from these. results and discussion once the data had been analysed it became apparent that μ was using a consistent intonation system which involved a number of idiosyncratic combinations of features. this is illustrated in detail below. the intonation system used by m: 1. tone units: table 1 summarises m's use of tone units. μ used a total of 567 complete, intelligible tone units, and 10 incomplete ones. the average length of the tone units was 3,7 words, and 81,1% of the tone units were between one and five words in length. crystal (1969) found that the average number of words per tone unit was five and that 80% of all tone units had lengths between one word and seven words. therefore, μ used shorter tone units on the whole, although the significance of the difference has not been established statistically. table 1: the use of tone units by μ number of complete intelligible tone units 567 total number of words 2157 average number of words per tone unit 3,7 percentage of tone units which ranged from 1 to 5 words 81,1% percentage of tone units which ranged from 6 to 10 words 18,9% number of minor sentence tone units 43 number of minor sentence tone units expressed as a percentage of the total 7,5 % number of incomplete tone units 10 it would seem that μ was in fact using too many tone units and as a result too many nuclear tones were marked. this overuse of nuclear tones, in turn, results in the loss of the functions of tonicity, namely to highlight new information and mark grammatical contrasts. the following are some examples of the excessive use of tone units. m: but we stayed in / a very simple / accommodation / m: it used to be / a stable / for horses / m: |"i've been tof dartmoor / 'once before / m: 'it's . fields / and country / — 1 all around / all the above examples would have been single tone units rather than separate ones in the speech of a hearing speaker with only the last item stressed or highlighted. μ is clearly not using tone units to delimit sense-groups. rather, she has segmented the sense groups inappropriately into phrases. a phonetic variation on the excessive use of tone units was the use of glissando which involves glides and also causes accentuation. for example: 'gliss' μ: i 'use it "mainly for 'going' to worlc/ 'gliss' m: say '"twenty-'five de'grees"f cefitigrade / in the above examples μ used glissando rather than separate tone units with a similar result. one way of overcoming the loss of functions of tonicity would be to devise an alternative means of marking these. in other words, it is possible that μ is using an idiosyncratic or abnormal way of marking the limits of sense-groups. in fact, the data suggest that μ used the tone system, rather than tone units, to serve this function. this hypothesis will be discussed in detail in the section on tone. another effect of m's excessive use of tone units is the fact that some tone units sound complete on an intonational level, but on a syntactic and semantic level they are incomplete. for example, in m: and the elms / t: and that's w h a t . . . 'gliss' m: a ''lot of elms / . 'get 'it / m's first utterance seems incomplete in all respects except for the presence of a tonic on "elms". the literature on the speech of the deaf makes reference to sentences being broken up into unusual breath-groups (hudgins and numbers, 1942, cited in stark, 1979), and to the presence of inappropriate pauses between words in mid-phrase (stark, 1979). a large amount of breath is used for each phrase thereby reducing the number of words or length of phrase per breath (nickerson and stevens, 1973). therefore it would seem that the use of too many tone units, (or to put it in another way, the use of tone units which are shorter than normal) seems to be a recognised feature of deaf speech. the explanation for this could be due to what ling (1976: 12) describes as "a lack of coordination between the articulators and the breath-voice system". 2. tonicity: tonic placement can be either final or non-final in the tone unit. out of a total of 395 tonic markings, 293 were final and 102 nonfinal. this represents 74% and 26% of the total respectively. tonic placement was only counted for those tone units which were larger than one word, and furthermore, it was decided to look at sense-groups rather than solely the tone units. this was felt to be justified as the excessive use of nuclear tones, not necessarily marking tonicity, would have yielded confusing results. | as mentioned above, it seems that μ uses an idiosyncratic system for marking prominence (see below for further discussion). ι as the above percentages indicate, μ used mostly final tonic placement. crystal (1969) suggests that the normal pattern is about 90% final and 10% non-final placement. therefore it would seem that μ followed the normal trend except that she tended to use non-final placements more than might have been expected. the following examples illustrate firstly, final and secondly, nonfinal tonic placement. 1. 'gliss' m: we 'went up on the "thursday' after'noon / 'gliss' m: — it was ''really a con'verted' stable / 'gliss' m: 'never came ''back the same' way / 2. m: we | "climbed . to the top hill / — m: 'they have the "highest welsh 'mountains / m: there are higher ones 'there / ^ μ did not use correct tonic placement in all cases. of the final placements, 76% were correct and 24% incorrect. some examples of incorrect final placement are given below. 3. 'gliss' m: and "three 'people' . | slept / on the |' two mattresses / 4. m: 'we went for | this 'easter weekend / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an international analysis of deaf speech: a case study 75 the underlined syllables indicate the tonic placement in question. in example 3 the tonic placement should have been on "three" and "two" to indicate the meaning contrast. there is no contrast meant between "slept" and "mattressess". in example 4 a more natural accent would have been to put the tonic placement on "this". example 3 demonstrates the grammatical function of tonicity where the placement of the tonic is determined by the preceding grammatical context. example 4 demonstrates the accentual function of tonicity which is independent of grammatical context. however, m's placement of the tonic does not show correct use of these functions. it seems therefore that μ has some but not full control of the rules governing tonic placement. of the non-final tonic placements, 81% were correct, 7% should have been final placements, and 12% were used correctly in that they were non-final but were placed on the wrong non-final item in the tone unit. μ: i 'went up to the 1 north t yorkshire dales / — in the above example "yorkshire" is made prominent when it need not be as no contrast is being made between "yorkshire dales" as opposed to some other "dales". the tonic placement would have been more appropriate on "dales" making it a final placement. an example where the use of non-final placement was correct but the non-final item chosen was incorrect, is the following: m: which "is / | "right up / — the 'north 'west part of 'yorkshire / in this utterance, if the non-final tonic placement is to be used, it would be more appropriate on "west" rather than "part", as the important information is the fact that it is the "north-west" and not the "north-east" part. a further example is m: y o u t 'don't . 'see anybody / 'gliss' we "hardly see any people / where the tonic should have been placed on "any" rather than "see" in the second utterance. m's use of tonicity mostly on lexical items rather than grammatical ones follows the normal pattern, in that 98% of tonic placements were on lexical items and 2% on grammatical ones. in crystal's (1969) data, 93% of tonics were placed on lexical items and 7% on grammatical ones. most of the placements of tonics on grammatical items in the data for| this project occurred in final position, and were generally correct, as the following examples show. 'gliss' m: we 'went up on the ''thursday 'after'noon / we 'drove up / infears / erm — 'gliss' it "took all 'afternoon' to getf up there / the tonic placement on the second "up" is quite appropriate in the context. the first "up" should not have a tonic and should be part of the same tone unit as "in cars". 'gliss' m: "where we' slept / 'gliss' 'was — "sectioned' off / — , in the above example, the placement of the tonic on "off" seems to fit correctly in the context. therefore, with regard to tonicity, the data indicate that μ is not fully in control of tonic placement. there is little literature concerning the placement of tonics by deaf speakers. stark (1979:239) in her summary of the features of deaf speech, mentions that 'word accents are misplaced and normally unaccented syllables are accented'. this statement refers to word stress rather than die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 sentence stress, the concern of tonicity. there were very few instances of incorrect word stress in the present data. two notable examples were the placement of the stress on the first rather than the second syllable of "certificate" and on the second syllable of "anyway" rather than the first. a later instance of "anyway" was produced with the correct stress. the lack of reference to tonicity in the literature on deaf speech reflects the fact that research has concentrated on the phonetic level and has not looked at the phonological use of these features. 3. tone: tonal aspects on intonation deal with the direction of pitch movement which occurs on the nucleus or tonic. crystal (1969) indicates that falling tones are the most commonly used, with rising tones being second in frequency of use. table 2 shows the distribution of tone types in m's speech. table 2: tone types in m's speech type number percentage of total falling \ full nuclear 82% sub-nuclear 18% 388 73% rising / full nuclear 81% sub-nuclear 19% 16 3% fall-rise v full nuclear 94% sub-nuclear 6% 86 16% fall + rise \ + / full nuclear 92% sub-nuclear 8% 42 8% in the above discussion it was suggested that μ had devised an idiosyncratic system to mark the limits of sense-groups. this was necessary because she used an excessive number of nuclear tones and thereby lost the ability to delimit sense-groups using tone unit boundaries. in analysing the placement of tonics in the data, the tone units were grouped into sense-groups, and the tonic for the sense-group, rather than for the individual tone units, was then determined. therefore the tones used by μ were classified as full nuclear when they were considered to be the nuclear tone of the sense-group, and sub-nuclear when they were not. crystal (1969) suggests that falling tones occur on about 51% of nuclei, and rising tones on about 20%. the other tone types seem to occur in relatively similar proportions. in the data for this subject falling tones were used far more and rising tones far less than expected. this could be a function of the sample type (i.e. informal conversation) as, for example, reading produces many more rising tones. falling tones are considered to be the most basic of the tones and are the first to be used by normal hearing children learning their mother tongue (crystal, 1979). it is also the falling tone which carries the least attitudinal information in english, being the neutral, unmarked form (crystal, 1969). therefore, if, due to the handicap of deafness, μ has not developed a full, normal intonation system, it is not surprising that the falling tone is far more common in the present data than expected. the following are some examples of full nuclear falling tones: r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 marguerite schneider 'gliss' m: — 'cause there's a "lot of 'other' traffic / — of the h61iday 'traffic / m: erm it's 'so quiet / m: — the 'whole building / 'gliss' m: a'bout — as "big as 'this' r6om / μ: i 'haven't. 'got a 'very good sleeping bag / i'm going to 'buy a | better one / l i t e r / the following examples show the use of falling tones in subnuclear positions: m: it was 'just 'like — you 'could 'imagine it / being a stible / m: the do6r / was . divided / in two / in the examples given below it will be noticed that the full nuclear tonic is in fact a falling tone produced with a narrow pitch range. m: f'that is about / 'gliss' "three and a half 'thousand feet' high / 'gliss' m: but we "get"' very 'wrapped'. 'warmly 'wrapped!ύρ / for reasons that will be discussed below, it is assumed that the narrowed falling tone is a variant of the falling tone, and that the narrowing could be seen as merely a phonetic realisation of the falling tone, just as one finds intra-person variations in phonemic productions. furthermore, narrowing can be related to a common observation in the literature that the speech of deaf speakers has a tendency to be monotonous (stark, 1979). if one did an analysis of the semantic contexts associated with the use of either variant, then it is possible that the falling and narrow falling tones might be used in different contexts. however, that is not within the scope of this paper. the tone types which occurred with most frequency after the falling tone were the fall-rise complex tone and fall + rise compound tone. both these tones were produced with a narrow pitch range on the rising component, not an entirely abnormal feature as crystal (1969) describes these tones as having a more prominent first component. the complex tone occurred about twice as often as the compound tone. a further look at the data indicates that comparison of m's use of these two tones with normal usage is not really valid as they seem to be used for different purposes. it was observed that i) the compound tone ( \ + / ) tended to occur predominantly as a nuclear tone in non-final position, ii) the complex tone (\ / ) tended to occur as a nuclear tone in final position, and iii) the use of the complex tone was much more frequent than the use of the compound tone (i.e. final tonic placement occurred more frequently than non-final placement). therefore, the distribution of these tones was analysed in more detail. the results show that, of the complex tones, 94% occurred as a full final nuclear tone, while only 6% occurred as sub-nuclear tones. furthermore, the tones which occurred in sub-nuclear position also marked final tonic placement within their tone units. the following examples show the use of this tone. m: wet work for people / 'gliss' like the "national trust / 'gliss' or — a "local' council / 'gliss' or a "nature' . council / 'gliss' m: a "lot of 'dead' elms / mi^'what we do / is vdjuntary / the first two examples show full nuclear tones and the third a subnuclear tone. (it should be noted that, in giving these examples, the correctness or incorrectness of the tonic placement is not at issue). similar results were observed with the occurrence of the compound tone, namely that 92% occurred as non-final full nuclear tones, while only 8% occurred as non-final sub-nuclear tones. the examples presented below demonstrate the use of the compound tone, the first two being non-final full nuclear tones and the last being a non-final sub-nuclear tone. m: 'there's no traffic 'warden / on saturdays / m: but i's 'very unlucky that afternoon / — m: it 'would've 'cost. 'about seven 'seventy pence / for the three hours i was . away / m: well i've parked my bike / there / m's purpose in using these tones seems to be to mark the end of sense-groups which normally would be marked by the use of tone unit boundaries. furthermore, μ uses the complex and compound tones for the same phonological function, thereby giving them the status of allophonic variations of the same tone in complementary distribution. however, μ does not only use the complex and compound tones to delimit sense-groups. she also uses the simple falling and narrow falling tones to fullfil this function. in the data, falling and narrow falling tones tended to be used for shorter sense-groups than the other tones, but this was not an important trend. finally, as table 2 indicates, μ used few instances of rising tones and no instances of complex and compound tones other than the fall-rise and fall + rise discussed above. therefore the use of tone types by μ has proved to be quite systematic in nature. however, it is not a normal system, in that μ seems to be using the three main tones of her system more as variations of one tone than as separate tones phonologically. this would indicate that μ has a very small range of tone types with which to mark meaning contrasts in terms of attitude. however, this assumption would need to be checked by analysing the semantic use of these tones; in other words, is μ marking different attitudes with these tones? it seems unlikely that she is, as her tone system functions to delimit sense-groups rather than to mark attitude contrasts. ; t in summary, the most important characteristics of m's intonation system are the following: | 1. an excessive number of tone units which do not correspond to the notion of tone units delimiting sense-groups; ι 2. an inconsistent ability to mark grammatical contrasts arid to highlight new information by means of varying tonic placement within the tone unit; 3. a very small tonal system which seems to be undertaking the function of delimiting sense-groups rather than marking attitude contrasts. the prediction is that, on a semantic level, μ will have a reduced ability to mark or express meaning contrasts usually indicated, in part, by variations in number of tone units, tonicity and tone. for example, crystal (1969) suggests that a haughty attitude is partly expressed by the use of non-simple tones, a puzzled attitude by a relatively high number of tone units and extremely stable tonicity and an amused one by a low number of simple falls. therefore it would be interesting to investigate the ability of deaf speakers to express these attitudes and the means by which they express them. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an international analysis of deaf speech: a case study 77 conclusion the intonation system which has emerged from the analysis of m's speech is deviant and deficient in nature. although μ has a number of normal features on a phonetic level, she does not use them in the normal manner to mark phonological contrasts. in that sense her system is deviant. the tone system used by μ includes basically one tone type with a number of phonetic variants, and in that way it is deficient. therapeutic intervention would aim at remediating both of these factors, using for example, visual display aids to teach different tone types and correct placement of the tonic. the aims of this paper have been achieved, and in addition, the hypothesis that was put forward has been shown to be correct for the speech of one congenitally hearing-impaired adult. one cannot generalise from one case, but it is not unlikely that other hearing-impaired speakers will use intonation systematically with a deviant and deficient system, although their individual systems might not have the same characteristics of m's system. an important factor arising from the analysis is the need to analyse the use of intonation not only in terms of the phonetic features but also in terms of how those features are used phonologically. in other words, assessment aimed at establishing therapeutic goals necessitates both a phonetic and phonological description of speech. this was highlighted by the fact that on a phonetic level, μ used at least three different tone types, but on a phonological level they functioned as a single tone. acknowledgements i wish to thank professor david crystal of the department of linguistic science, university of reading, reading, england, for his help and guidance in the analysis of the data. references bruner, j. the ontogenesis of speech acts,j of child lang. 2,1-19, 1975. crystal, d. prosodic systems and intonation in english, cup, 1969. crystal, d. prosodic development, in fletcher, p. and garman, m. (eds) language acquisition, cup, 1979. dodd,b. the phonological system of deaf children, j: speech hear disord., 41, 185-198, 19[76. john, j.e.j, and howarth, j.n. the effects of time distortions on ι the intelligibility of deaf children's speech, lang, and speech 8, 127-134, 1965. [ king, a. and parker, a. the relevance of prosodic features to speech work with hearing-impaired children, in jones, t.m. (ed) language disability in children, assessment and remediation, mip press ltd. 1980. king, α., parker, α., spanner, m. and wright, r.a. speech display computer for use in schools for the deaf, royal national institute for the deaf, london, 1982. ling, d. speech and the hearing impaired child: theory and practice, washington: the alex. graham bell assoc. for the deaf, 1976. martony, j. on the correction of voice pitch level for severely hard of hearing subjects, amer. annals of the deaf, 113,195202, 1968. nickerson, r.s. and stevens, k.n. teaching speech to the deaf: can a computer help? ieee transactions on audio, and electroacoustics, au-21. 5, 1973. nickerson, r.s., kalikon, d.n. and stevens, k.n. computeraided speech training for the deaf, j. speech hear disord., 41, 120-132, 1976. phillips, n.d., remillard, w., bass, s. and pronovost, w. teaching of intonation to the deaf by visual pattern matching, amer. annals of the deaf. 113, 239-246, 1968. silverman, s.r. and calvert, d.r. conservation and development of speech, in davis, h. and silverman, s.r. (eds) hearing and deafness, 4th ed, holt, rinehart and winston, 1978. stark, r, speech of the hearing-impaired child, in bradford, l.j. and hardy, w.g. (eds) hearing and hearing-mpairment, grune and stratton, 1979. appendix a: symbols used for the transcription \ = falling tone / = rising tone v / = fall-rise tone \ + / = fall + rise tone η = narrow pitch range = short pause — longer pause ' o r 1 1 = stress (extra loudness as opposed to pitch movement) t = raise in pitch 'gliss' " '' = glissando / / = tone unit boundary syll = tonic syllable referred to in examples die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 103 editing a scientific journal on communication disorders in south africa: a unique challenge* brenda louw department of communication pathology university of pretoria abstract the aim of this article is to describe the challenge of editing a scientific journal in the field of communication disorders in the south african context. an overview of the development of the journal and the current editorial policy is described. the distinctiveness and uniqueness of the journal is highlighted. a discussion of the problems encountered and the challenges posed by the future is supplied. in conclusion the question is posed of whether a scientific publication of this nature is relevant and justifiable. it is concluded that such a publication is indeed important and has the right of existence. opsomming die doel van die artikel is om die uitdaging te beskryf wat die redaksie van 'n wetenskaplike tydskrif in die vakgebied van kommunikasiepatologie in die suid-afrikaanse konteks hied, 'n oorsig van die geskiedkundige ontwikkeling van die tydskrif en die huidige beleid word bespreek. die onderskeidende en andersoortige aard van die tydskrif word uitgelig enprobleme in die redaksie en produksie van die tydskrif word ge'identifiseer. uitdagings en moontlike oplossings vir die bestaande probleme word aangespreek. ten slotte word die relevansie van 'n wetenskaplike publikasie van hierdie aard bevraagteken. die gevolgtrekking word bereik dat so 'n publikasie wel bestaansreg het. writing and publication are important in developing the scholarly base of speech-language therapy and audiology. scholarly journals serve as archives of research data, contain literature reviews, and other types of materials that contribute to the scientific base of a discipline or that have broad and lasting clinical relevance. because knowledge is ever-changing and advancing, the facts and findings published at any one point in time may become dated or proven incorrect. nevertheless, the hallmark of a scholarly publication is that it represents the current state of art or science at the time of publication (asha, 1993). the practice of the profession of speech-language therapy and audiology in south africa has been reflected and supported by it's own scholarly journal over the years. the editing of this scientific journal poses unique challenges to the editor. the aim of this article is to describe these challenges by supplying an overview of the south african journal of communication disorders, describing the distinctiveness and uniqueness of the journal, identifying the problems encountered and providing possible solutions in meeting these problems. the aim of the south african journal of communication disorders is to publish papers or reports concerned with research, and critically evaluative theoretical and philosophical conceptual issues dealing with aspects of human communication and its disorders, service provision, training and policy. it serves as the scientific mouthpiece for local members of the profession and mirrors the professional and theoretical trends within the local context by publishing both research and review articles. a historical overview indicates that the profession of speech-language-therapy and audiology has been practiced in south africa since the late 1930's. prof. pierre de villiers pienaar initiated the training of speech-language-therapists and audiologists at the university of the witwatersrand in 1937, and the course is currently taught at five universities in south africa (uys, 1993). the south african journal of communication disorders was first published by the south african speech, language, hearing association (saslha) in 1953 and the fortieth volume appeared in 1993. the journal is published annually. throughout the forty years of publication the journal has undergone three title changes, from "journal of the logopaedic society" to "journal of the south african speech and hearing association" to the current title, reflecting changes within * this article is based on a paper of the same title presented at the international conference on biomedical periodicals, beijing, china, june 16-18, 1994. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 104 the field and the association. the current title reflects the underlying paradigmatic shift of the profession in the south african context. the editorial policy is published in every issue and employs the peer review system (strayhorn, mcdermott & tanquay, 1993). peer review has become an important aspect of quality control in scientific publications. peer reviewers are experts in the topic of a submitted manuscript who are considered "peers" of the authors, and who are selected by the editor to assess the quality of the manuscript's scientific and technical content. the editor remains responsible for decisions regarding rejection, revision, or acceptance of publication (glass, 1994). this process of refereeing, or editorial peer review, increases the credibility of a journal by assuring that only scientifically sound research is published (weller, 1987). although the south african journal of communication disorders is comparable to other journals in the field published in the united kingdom, australia and the united states of america, it is distinctly different from these publications and has a unique character. the distinctiveness and uniqueness of the journal is embedded in the fact that two distinct, yet integrated, fields of speech-language pathology and audiology in which training is done, are represented by the journal, leading to diverse topics and research methodologies reflecting the differing nature of the two fields. in addition articles are published in the two (up till 1993) current official languages of south africa, english and afrikaans, thus rendering the journal to be bilingual. south africa represents a multilingual, multicultural society and articles increasingly reflect the diversity of the communication disorders and underlying issues encountered in this context. circulation figures of the journal are limited (approximately 800) which is ascribed to the fact that there were 980 speech-language therapists/audiologists registered with the south african medical and dental council in 1993, of whom approximately 650 are members of saslha and subscribe to the journal as part of their membership. this differs greatly from, for example, the membership figures of the american speechlanguage hearing association (aslha), with an approximate membership of 70 000 (asha, 1994). members of aslha selectively subscribe to the various journals published by their association. lastly, the uniqueness of the journal is related to geographical and political factors, as south africa has been relatively isolated from international contact (aron, 1991). this has led to limited international contributions and has characterized the journal with a unique south african perspective. it is therefore clear that the journal is distinct from others published in the field internationally, and in order to retain this uniqueness and to be truly representative of the context in which it appears, the editor is faced with exciting challenges. the challenges facing the editor of the south african journal of communication disorders are twofold, those relating to the general problems of scholarly journals and those stemming from the distinctiveness of the journal. according to leslie (1989) scholarly journals have numerous problems. although many scientific journals the south brenda louw are published, only very few can be labelled as quality journals due to various reasons, e.g., some lack central focus, others are too focussed and reject submissions if the content does not fit prescribed research agenda. a central problem to scholarly journals is manuscript review (leslie, 1989). the need for peer review is generally conceded, but the operation of the system is fraught with controversies e.g., conflicts of interest, importance of articles, masking of identities during peer review, and reviewer bias (grassman, 1986; siegelman & whicker, 1987; glass, 1994). leslie (1989) expressed the opinion that the time to examine these problems, and to search for an implement solutions is at hand. he poses the following question: do we have the courage to begin this important work? (leslie, 1989, p. 128). the answer is yes, as these problems are being addressed internationally by conferences, task groups and public actions in order to improve the quality of journals and to develop the editing process of scholarly journals into a profession (glass, 1994). when confronted by leslie's (1989) question and international developments, it is clear that the problems of the south african journal of communication disorders need to be identified and solutions proposed to ensure the continuation of the forty year old tradition of the journal. in response to these challenges and developments, the author who is the current editor of the journal, conducted a critical appraisal of the publication process of the the south african journal of communication disorders and of literature relating to editorial and publication issues. the following eightpro&iems were identified and addressed. many of the problems relate to the distinctive nature of the journal, as discussed earlier. firstly the language in which articles are written poses a problem. for example, articles written in afrikaans are not accessible to all subscribers. to write a scientific manuscript in a second language is problematic and professional translation is costly and does not usually convey the exact meaning of the author. imminent political changes, however, may designate english as one of the official languages which could necessitate changes in editorial policy. i the number of speech-language therapists and|audiologists from different linguistic and cultural backgrounds is increasing and will hopefully lead to anj increased research on communication disorders in the various cultural and linguistic groups as well as in intercultural research, which is urgently needed in the field. designating the publication of all articles in one language, namely english, will make information reasonably accessible to most professionals in the field, as opposed to publishing articles in the author's first language. abstracts may then be published in the author's first language or in a chosen second language. this implies that the editor would need to make use of additional resources in editing the second language abstracts, but would emphasize the distinctive nature of the journal. secondly, although south africa represents a multilingual, multicultural society and poses unique challenges to service delivery, the south african journal of communication disorders has not adequately developed χ ican journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ing a scientific journal on communication disorders in south africa: a unique challenge a distinct local character. the journal has been modelled on journals published by the american speech-language hearing association. a possible solution could be to increase the journal's scholarly impact by specifically developing the multilingual, multicultural aspects (as mentioned above) and developing the perspective of the profession contextualized in an intersectoral, transdisciplinary context of an integrated health care system. such an approach would provide a truly distinctive character to the journal. it could also contribute to the procuring of funding if the journal took on a new, strongly developed unique character, with a definite transformatory human rights perspective. this is consistent with the theoretical paradigm underlying the discipline, namely that the communication of an individual is always viewed in the context of social systems. thirdly, few professionals are willing to serve on the editorial board. professionals appear to be willing to act as reviewers, but not to become involved on any other level of management of the journal. this may be attributed to the "burn-out" phenomena within the profession (swidler & ross, 1993). according to weller (1987) journal quality is affected by the editorial staff. the implications of a limited editorial board are an increase in the editor's responsibilities regarding the peer review process, placement decisions, and the lack of a support mechanism on both a technical and emotional level. a proposed solution to this pressing problem is that the editor actively recruits members for an extended editorial board by clearly stating the advantages of being involved in the publication process of a scholarly journal. master degree level students could be motivated to serve as editorial staff members by expounding the benefits of being actively involved in the development of the data basis of the field of speech-language pathology and audiology. lastly, the editor can approach employers to recognize the importance of and benefits of employees participating in the editorial process of the journal, by crediting the individuals for promotion purposes, research grant or subsidy applications. fourthly, as mentioned previously, the circulation figures of the journal are limited, implying small financial gain and reader audience] increasing circulation figures may serve as a solution to both financial problems and the reader audience. this can be affected by recruiting student members and an active public relations drive of the saslha and the journal!itself. increased international contact may encourage new contributors, but the relevance of such contributions and the unique local character of the journal must not be compromised. fifthly, given the limited circulation figures, it follows that the source of contributors to the journal is also very limited. five universities offer undergraduate training in the field, thus contributors from the academia are a small group. relatively few professionals in clinical practice contribute to the journal, mainly due to two reasons. the workload created by the imbalance that exists between professional and communicatively disordered in the south african context (uys, 1993), allows for little time allocated to conducting scientific research and the writing of articles. secondly, only a small corps of professionals in the field actively conduct research, and even fewer are prepared to devote time to writing articles on conceptual issues related to the fields of speech-language pathology and audiology. a viable solution is to publish articles based on outstanding student research reports. this not only reflects the current development and interest at the various training centres, but also makes interesting research findings available and contributes to the data base of local research. in giving this guidance to student researchers the journals are contributing to a genre of graduates with the skills and attitudes relevant to a "culture" of publishing. according to silverman (1988) clinical research needs to be encouraged as it is beneficial to both the communicatively handicapped population and to members of the profession. the communicatively handicapped population can benefit by the publication of clinical research results as the sharing of information by professionals leads to higher quality services. professionals benefit from conducting clinical research as it allows them to satisfy a requirement of the profession's code of ethics; reduces "burn-out"; and improves clinical effectiveness (silverman, 1988). the questions, answers and implications of empirical and clinical research must, however, be communicated to potential consumers before they can have impact on the quality of services for the communicatively handicapped. according to silverman (1988) reporting information in professional journals is the most effective manner to reach large audiences. different kinds of incentives can be created by institutions to encourage clinical investigations and clinical researchers can be accommodated to enable them to conduct research. the editorial board of the journal can take the initiative and approach institutions in this regard to ensure a constant flow of articles to be submitted for publication. according to yoder wise (1992) writing is a professional commitment that requires planning and dedication. scholarly journals need to strive to help authors in their development of manuscripts in order to create an important body of knowledge in a discipline (yoder wise, 1992). sixthly, the peer review process employed by the journal poses many problems. according to yoder wise (1992) publication in a peer-reviewed journal should be a mark of distinction. this system is, however, fraught with problems (siegelman & wicker, 1987; oxman, 1991; abdellah, 1990). two striking aspects of peer review are that it is based almost entirely on uncompensated voluntary work, and that the peer review system itself has only recently come under scientific scrutiny. although peer review is viewed as a major facet of the evaluation of science, it is subject to human error. this pillar of science merits scientific research to refine the process and to develop a stricter application of scientific standard (glass, 1994). the editorial board thus needs to keep abreast with recent international trends of article reviewing and select peer reviewers with care to ensure that the peer review system is practiced optimally (leslie, 1989; strayhorn, mcdermott & tanquay, 1993; lightdale, 1992; grassmann, 1986). in addition to the problems of peer review as identified in the literature, the context in which this journal is published, leads to additional problems such as the limited corps of reviewers available and the fact that not all reviewers are able to effectively review a scientific article in a second language. certain criteria are, however, used as an aid to the peer review process in an attempt to increase the creddie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 106 brenda louw ibility of the journal by assuring that only scientifically sound research and review articles of scientific quality are published (oxman, 1991; weller, 1987). seventhly, funding for the journal is problematic due to the current financial climate in south africa and the limited sources of saslha. the journal receives a grant from the foundation for education, science and technology with saslha having to bear the brunt of the costs. funding determines the continuation of a publication to a large extent and alternatives will need to be found if the tradition of the journal is to be continued. recruiting an increased number of advertisements could increase the journal's income to assist in publication costs. the financial challenge is partially met by desk top publishing, which necessitates disk copies of the submitted articles and ensuring that the articles which are accepted for publication, are print ready. printing from "print ready" software reduces the printing costs dramatically and shortens the printing process. desk top printing, however, creates another editorial problem. in the south african context, papers tend to be submitted in grossly unedited forms. due to the limited source, mentioned earlier, competition is minimal and papers are accepted and then have to go through a long editorial process before being "print ready". a possible solution to this problem may be the expansion of the information for contributors which is contained in each issue of the journal and strict adherence to these guidelines by the authors. the editor should also retain the right to refuse publication of an accepted article if the set requirements of editorial care are not met.' eighthly, the post of editor of the south african journal of communication disorders is a voluntary one and financially uncompensated, which implies that the editor practices this portfolio in a part-time capacity, while usually employed in another full-time post. the demands of editing are great and according to weller (1987) the quality of a journal is affected by the continuity of good editors. in order to meet these challenges scholarly journals must provide editors with sufficient resources to do their jobs. according to leslie (1989, p. 126) the solution is to have the journal sponsor, whether a professional, academic, research organization or university (or a combination of these) provide editors with time, money, personnel, and equipment. if the publication of scholary work is important, it deserves this kind of commitment (leslie, 1989). the editor must actively campaign for these benefits in order to function appropriately. effective editorial management can make an important contribution to the continuity and quality of the journal. maintaining quality and standards of the journal in the given circumstances requires a concentrated effort to continue the international recognition the journal currently enjoys and to diligently carry out administration adequately so as to maintain international listing and abstracting of the journal. according to weller (1987) the quality of a journal is determined by the following factors the editorial staff, contributors, and refereeing process, the visual aspects of the journal and the continuity of good editors and authors (weller, 1987). by being aware of and addressing these contributing factors, the journal editor can function in an accountable fashion and ensure a quality product. despite the wide range of problems identified in publishing the south african journal of communication disorders, it appears that many solutions can be implemented to minimize, and even overcome these deterrents to a successful publication. conclusion: the ultimate challenge posed to the editor of the south african journal of communication disorders is whether a scientific publication in the fields of speechlanguage pathology and audiology is at all relevant in the south african situation? the alternative to a scientific publication is a more informal dissemination of clinical information. according to silverman (1989) this can be done by means of newsletters, study groups and informal communication between clinicians. another possible solution could be to abandon the discipline of specific journals that tend to lead to fragmentation in the south african context. instead, other disciplines such as e.g., occupational therapy, physiotherapy, psychology, general medical practice, could be involved to promote the concept of a transdisciplinary approach in meeting the needs of the people in an integrated health care system. based on the following argument it is concluded that the publication of the south african journal of communication disorders is indeed important and needs to be continued in the future. publishing articles in a peerreviewed journal is a mark of distinction. the time and effort invested in the publication process by the author, the reviewers and the editor is justified to help create an important body of knowledge in the field (yoder wise, 1992). this is specifically true of the south african situation in which a dirth of knowledge still exists regarding communication disorders and service delivery in the local context. research on the needs of the culturally and linguistically diverse population of south africa is needed in order to make clinical applications and to increase the effectiveness and accountability of service delivery. by publishing articles on issues dealing with communication disorders, service provision, training and policy the scientific journal supports the profession of speech-language pathology and audiology in taking responsibility for shaping its own future (cole, 1986; uys, 1993). j i acknowledgements the author wishes to thank glen jager, previbus editor of the south african journal of communication disorders, and prof. rene hugo for their valuable contribution in peer-reviewing this article. the financial assistance received from the centre for science development, hsrc and the university of pretoria is acknowledged with appreciation. references abdellah, f.g. 1990. peer review the only answer to highquality research? journal of professionalnursing, 6, pp 70-75. / aron, m.l. (1991). perspectives. the sokth african journal of communication disorders, 38, pp'. 3-12. ' asha. (1993). the publication process. a guide for authors. asha, 37, pp 142-144. asha. (1994). changing courses midstream. asha, 38, pp 38-39. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e d i t i n g a scientific j o u r n a l o n c o m m u n i c a t i o n d i s o r d e r s i n s o u t h africa: a u n i q u e c h a l l e n g e 107 cole, p.r. (1986). i want to shape my own future. how about you? asha, 28, pp. 41-42. glass, r.m. (1994). peer review and quality control in biomedical p u b l i c a t i o n . international conference on biomedical periodicals, abstracts, p. 1. grassmann, w.k. (1986). is the fact that the emperor wears no clothes a subject worthy of publication? interfaces, 16, pp 42-51. leslie l.z. (1989). manuscript review: a view from below. scholarly publishing, 20 pp. 123-128. lightdale, c.j. (1992). the editor : professor or journalist? gastrointestinal endoscopy, 38, pp 193-194. o x m a n , a.d. (1991). agreement among reviewers of review articles. journal of clinical epidemiology 44, pp. 91-98. siegelman, l. & whicker, m.l. (1987). some implications of bias in peer review: a simulation based analysis. social science quarterly, 68, pp. 494-509. s i l v e r m a n , f.h. (1988). speech language pathology and audiology. columbus: c ε merrill publ. co. strayhorn, j., mc dermott & j.f. tanquay, p. (1993). an intervention to improve the reliability of manuscript reviews for the journal of the american academy of child and adolescent psychiatry. american journal of psychiatry, 150 (6), pp. 947-952. swidler, m. & ross, e. (1993). burn-out: a smouldering problem among south-african s p e e c h l a n g u a g e pathologists? the south african journal of communication disorders, 40, pp. 71-84. uys, i.c. (1993). kommunikasiepatologie: onderrig vir die toekoms. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 40, pp. 3-10. weller, a.c. (1987). editorial policy and the assessment of quality among medical journals. bulletin of medical library association, 75, pp. 310-316. yoder wise, p. (1992). from manuscript to publication. journal of continuing education in nursing, 23, p. 51. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, and critically evaluative theoretical and philosophical conceptual issues dealing with aspects of human communication and its disorders, service provision, training and policy. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. manuscript style and requirements manuscripts should be accompanied by a covering letter providing the author's address and telephone numbers. all contributions are required to follow strictly, the style specified in the publication manual of the american psychological assoc. (3rd ed., 1983χαρα pub. man.), with complete internal consistency. four copies of triple-spaced high quality type-written manuscripts with numbered pages, and wide margins should be submitted. they should be accompanied by one identical disc copy of the paper; (1) in wordperfect 5.1 (with an extension .wp5). filenames should include the first author's initials and a clearly identifiable keyword or abbreviation thereof and should be typewritten on the last line of the last page of the reference list (for retrieval purposes only). as a rule, contributions should not exceed much more than 30 pages, although longer papers will be accepted if the additional length is warranted. the first page of two copies should contain the title of the article, name of author(s), and institutional affiliation (or address). in accordance with the αρα pub. man. style (1833, p.23) authors are not required to provide qualifications. in the remaining two copies, the first page should contain only the title. the second page of all copies, should contain only an abstract (100 words), written in english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors. major headings where applicable should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion acknowledgements, references. all paragraphs should be indented. tables and figures which should be prepared on separate sheets (one per page), should be copied for review purposes and only the copies sent initially. figures, graphs, and line drawings that are used for publication, however, must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper, but these will not be required until after the author has been notified of the acceptance of the article. lettering appearing on these should be uniform and professionally done, allowing for a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should appear below it and should not be included in the illustration. the titles of tables, which appear above, and figures, which appear below, should be concise but explanatory. both should be numbered in arabic numerals in order of appearance. the number of illustrative materials allowed, will be at the discretion of the editor (usually about 6). references references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and the date, e.g., van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, after the first occurrence, et al. after the first author will suffice, except for six or more when et al. may be used from the start. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list, which should be listed in strict alphabetical order in triple spacing at the end of the article. all references should be included in the list, including secondary sources, (αρα pub. man. 1983, p.13). only acceptable abbreviations of journals may be used, (see dsh abstracts, october; or the world list of scientific periodicals). the number of references should not exceed much more than 30, unless specifically warranted. examples locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear disord 48 339-341. penrod, j.p. (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimorewilliams & wilkins. davis, g.a. & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca: college-hill. editing acceptable manuscripts may be returned to the author for revision. additional minor changes may also be made at this stage, but a note on the manuscript acknowledging each alteration made by the author, is required. the paper is then returned to the editorial committee for final editing for style, clarity and consistency. reprints: 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. j ι deadline for contributions: the preferred date ii the 31st may each year, but papers will be accepted until 30th june by arrangement. j queries, correspondence & manuscripts: shiuld be addressed to the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech-language-hearing association, p.o. box 600, wits, 2050, south africa. 1 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) inligting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer verslae en artikels wat gemoeid is met navorsing, of handel oor krities evaluerende, teoretiese en filosofiese konseptuele kwessies wat oor menslike kommunikasie en kommunikasieafwykings, diensverskaffing, opleiding en beleid gaan. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings sal nie artikels aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. manuskrip styl en vereistes: m a n u s k r i p t e behoort deur 'n dekkingsbrief vergesel te word wat die skrywer se adres en telefoonnommers bevat. daar word van alle bydraers verwag om die styl, soos gespesifiseer is in die "publication manual of the american psychological assoc. (3rd ed., 1983) (αρα pub. man."), nougeset te volg met volledige interne ooreenstemming. manuskripte moet getik, van hoe gehalte en in drievoud spasiering met wye kantlyne wees. vier kopiee van die manuskrip moet verskaf word. een hiervan moet 'n identiese skyfkopie van die artikel wees in wordperfect 5.1 (met 'n uitbreiding .wp5). leername behoort die eerste skrywer se voorletters en 'n duidelike identifiseerbare sleutelwoord of afkorting daarvan in te sluit en moet op die laaste lyn van die bladsy van die verwsyingslys getik word (slegs vir naslaan doeleindes). as 'n reel moet bydraes nie 30 bladsye oorskry nie, maar langer artikels sal aanvaar word indien die addisionele lengte dit regverdig. op die eerste bladsy van twee van die afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, naam van die skrywer(s), en instansie (of adres) verskyn. in ooreenstemming met die "αρα pub. man." se styl word daar nie van skrywers verwag om enige kwalifikasies te verskaf nie. op die eerste bladsy van die twee oorblywende afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskaf word. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. hoofopskrifte moet, waar van toepassing, in die volgende volgorde verskaf word: j metode, resultate, besprekings, gevolgtrekkings, erkennings en verwysings. alle paragrawe moet ingekeep word. tabelle en figure wat op afsonderlike bladsye (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie) moet verskyn, moet vir referent-doeleindes gekopieer word en slegs die kopiee moet inisieel verskaf word. figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge wat vir publikasie gebruik word, moet egter oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte wees. die oorspronklikes sal slegs verlang word nadat die artikel vir publikasies aanvaar is. letterwerk wat op bogenoemde verskyn, moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50% verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by illustrasies moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verklarings of legendes moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maar daaronder, verskyn. die opskrifte van tabelle (wat bo-aan verskyn), en die onderskrifte van figure, (wat onderaan verskyn), moet beknop, maar verklarend wees. numering moet deur middel van arabiese syfers geskied. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word. die aantal tabelle en illustrasies wat ingesluit word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). verwysings verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, b.v., van riper (1971). wanneer daar egter meer as twee skrywers is, moet daar na die eerste verskaffing van al die outeurs, van et al. gebruik gemaak word. in die geval waar daar egter ses of meer outeurs ter sprake is moet et al. van die begin af gebruik word. al die name van die skrywers moet in die verwysingslys verskyn wat aan die einde van die artikel voorkom. verwysings moet alfabeties in trippel spasiering gerangskik word. al die verwysings moet in die verwysingslys verskyn, insluitende sekondere bronne, ("αρα pub. man." 1983, p. 13). slegs aanvaarbare afkortings van tydskrifte se titels mag gebruik word, (sien "dsh abstracts, october"; of the world list of scientific periodicals"). die aantal verwysings moet nie meer as 30 oorskry nie, tensy dit geregverdig is. let op ble volgende voorbeelde: locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341. penrod, j.p. (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. davis, g.a. & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca.: college-hill. redigering manuskripte wat aanvaar is, mag na die skrywer teruggestuur word vir hersiening. addisionele kleiner veranderinge mag ook op hierdie stadium aangebring word, maar 'n nota ter aanduiding van alle veranderinge wat op die manuskrip voorkom, moet verskaf word. die artikel word dan aan die redaksionele komitee vir finale redigering van styl, duidelikheid en konsekwentheid teruggestuur. herdrukke: 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis aan die outeurs verskaf word. sluitingsdatum vir bydraes: bydraes word verkieslik teen 31 mei elke jaar verwag, maar artikels sal nog tot 30 junie vir aanvaarding oorweeg word. navrae, korrespondensie en manuskripte: moet geadresseer word aan die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, die suid-afrikaanse spraak-taal-gehoor vereniging, posbus 600, wits 2050, suidafrika. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 53 the development of a model for geodesic learning: the geodesic information processing model caroline m. leaf, brenda louw and isabel uys department of communication pathology and audiology university of pretoria abstract the perception of ̂ arnin^n order* copew learning how to learn and self-directed inquiry as essential life-skills for the speech-language therapist are discussed. opsomming hierdie artikel voer aan dat alternatiewe benaderings tot die huidige tradisionele leermetodesessensieel isindier^nderriginhgtingsontplotting te han rnnteen self-gerigte navraag as essensiele lewensvaardigheid wat sisteme, * bewerkstellig in respons op veranderende situasies m p z z p me t s ^ m ^ p ^ z alternatiewe diensleweringsmodel, naamlik die geodetiese inligtingsproses^ r ^ ^ w ^ ^ j z van geodetiese filosofie val. die implikasies van hierdie alternatiewe benadermg vir die spraak-taalterapeut word ten slotte bespreek. key words: geodesic learning, mind mapping approach introduction j humans are biologically" designed to survive and the single greatest competitive advantage is the ability to learn (jensen, 1995: iv). traditional perceptions of learning the ability to learn, individually, in groups, in organisations and as a country, is a critical factor in the progress and development of society as a whole. traditionally, definitions of learning have been based on behaviouristic, mechanistic and cognitive theories (glasser, 1986; knowles, 1990). this has led to the assumption that learning is the internalisation of external knowledge, and is under the control of a single internal source of self-regulation, namely executive self-regulation (iran-nejad, 1990). this viewpoint defines learning as a growth process dependent on internalising events into a "storage" system that corresponds to the environment (knowles, 1990). therefore, most traditional learning theorists view learning as a process by which behaviour is changed, shaped or controlled, with an emphasis on growth and cognitive development (knowles, 1990; glasser, 1986). however, these assumptions undermine the creative and multimodal nature of learning, limiting learning to the simple incremental learning of facts and definitions, which in turn is responsible for the achievement and motivational problems many students experience (glasser, 1986; iran-nejad, 1990; gardner, 1985; knowles, 1990; jensen, 1995). it appears that behaviouristic and cognitive theories are too narrow to explain the complexity of the learning process as their primary focus is on concept attainment to the exclusion of concept formation or invention (glasser, 1986; jones, 1968, in knowles, 1990). furthermore, it needs to be acknowledged that that everyone learns in different ways, and that it is consequently necessary to explore alternative die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 ways of facilitating learning that will allow individuals to realise their natural potential. current traditional learning systems that propose to stimulate learning in the "whole" child, tend to favour predominantly linguistic and mathematical intelligences as opposed to a more pluralistic approach (jensen, 1995). this is a consequence of the prevailing philosophy of current traditional educational systems and therapeutic institutions which indicates that "intelligence" or "potential" is a single general capacity that enables the individual to achieve in all situations. research has, however, indicated that the traditional type of approach to developing learning potential , which is based on the i.q., piagetian and information processing movements, is product versus process oriented and does not develop the whole person in their full capacity (irannejad, 1990; gardner, 1985; jensen, 1995). many educators and philosophers concur that the most important goal of therapy and education is to facilitate thinking. hence an approach to learning is needed that moves away from viewing learning as a process of controlling, changing or shaping behaviour, to one that views learning as competency development. consequently, an alternative approach that focuses on the dynamics of the thought processes and the pluralistic nature of the intelligences of the human mind has arisen, and which is termed the geodesic movement (leaf, 1997; gardner, 1985; iran-nejad, 1990). the paradigm shift in speech-language therapy and audiology the paradigm shift that is occurring in the perception of learning is parallelled in the field of speech-language therapy and audiology, and has resulted in a paradigm shift in the professional self-concept and role played by the speech-language therapist. this paradigm shift has emerged in response to the increasing awareness of the inefficiency of traditional approaches in meeting needs of clients, with alternative service delivery models proposed to provide a more accountable service to clients (paulbrown, 1992; simon, 1987; leaf, uys & louw, 1990; lewis 1994). one of the communication needs stressed in the alternative service delivery literature is the need to integrate communication skills with academic material with the emphasis on the learning process (paul-brown, 1992). this is due to the academic environment requiring competent communication skills both oral and written as prerequisites for school success (johnson, 1987). a student's successes and failures are bound up in the way they share and create meaning through language. speech-language therapists therefore need to broaden their role to promote overall learning success, and in this way, provide a more accountable service to clients. speech-language therapists need to become more involved in facilitating the language and communication skills needed in the learning process in the classroom. this change therefore implies an evolution. according to johnson (1987:225), "the evolution from "speech-language therapist" to "communication and learning instructor" has been the result of adopting an educational versus a medical model, through integrating communication instruction into the students natural learning environment, and through collaborating with other educators". this entails the use of classroom and curriculum based service delivery models where the basis for the content of treatment would be the the south caroline m. leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw concepts and vocabulary from the academic curriculum. this implies a consultative role for the speech-language therapists allowing for their background and abilities to be utilised to a greater extent. not only can speech-language therapists provide direct therapy, but they can also provide input on the communication and social difficulties exhibited by the pupils as observed in the naturalistic learning environments, as well as about the process of language and learning in general (thurman & widerstrom, 1990). humanism versus behaviourism the development of humanistic psychology (founded in 1963) carries this trend of thought further in that the image of man is recast from a passive, reactive recipient to an active, seeking, autonomous and reflective being (rogers, 1969, in knowles, 1990). according to rogers (1969, in knowles), learning is seen as having a quality of personal involvement; as being self-initiated; as pervasive; as evaluated by the learner; and finally, as having meaning as its essence. this view is expanded by maslow (1970, in knowles, 1990) who identifies the goal of learning to be self-actualisation. jourard argues that "the learner has the need and the capacity to assume responsibility for his own continuing learning" (1972, in knowles, 1990). this humanistic view of learning has been formulated into a theory by glasser (1986) which he calls "learning control theory". learning control theory is a biological theory of how humans function as living creatures. it has as its basic premise the contention that all behaviour is an attempt to satisfy needs that are built into the genetic structure of the brain, and thus all motivation is internal, as opposed to external as claimed by behaviourists and cognitists. control theory contends that it is impossible to force or bribe a person into doing quality work. that is, learning is not a process of shaping change in behaviour, rather it is an internally motivated creation of meaning (glasser, 1986). iran-nejad (1990) elaborates on this idea by defining learning as the creative reconceptualisation of internal knowledge. he further proposes that there are two different sources of internal self-regulation; one that controls the sequential conscious aspect of learning,! and another that controls the simultaneous non-conscious aspect. furthermore, to extend the domain of learning beyond simple incremental memorisation, both sources of self-regulation have to be activated. ' ! both the behaviouristic and cognitive theories, wliich utilise computers and mechanistic processes as analogies, define learning as a change in behaviour that is largely controlled by an external source, and that will result in an accrual of knowledge facts. by contrast, humanistic theories, which have the functioning of the human brain as their analogy, define learning as an internally motivated and controlled process that results in the recreation of conceptual knowledge with the emphasis on meaning. behaviouristic and cognitive theories emphasise the educator, the agent of change who presents stimuli, and reinforcement for learning and designs activities to induce change. a humanistic approach, by contrast, emphasises the person in whom the change occurs, and learning as the act or process by which behavioural change, knowledge skills and attitude are reconceptualised. in this, a humanistic approach to learning appears to be a more accurate description of human functioning than a behavy african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a model for geodesic learning: the geodesic information processing model 55 iouristic and cognitive approach. extensive research has been conducted in the fields of contemporary neuroscience and neuropsychology and has led to the identification of the brain's preferred way to learn, confirming the latter statement (glasser, 1986; gardner, 1985; iran-nejad, 1990; jensen, 1995; knowles, 1990; 1986; lozanov, 1978; dhority, 1991; springer & deutsch, 1989; diamond, 1988). however, behaviouristic and cognitive theories tend to dominate the philosophy of learning institutions with what are believed to be negative effects on the learning abilities of students and clients and the realisation of their potential. the adoption of predominantly behaviouristic and cognitive philosophies is possibly due to a lack of integration between research on the brain and standard education practices. in addition, the behaviouristic and cognitive theories provide neat ways of "measuring" and "packaging" students and clients into controlled environments, and are thus convenient to educationalists and learning institutions. the humanistic approach recognises the complexity and individuality of human nature, and consequently the complex and involved task of facilitating learning. this approach is not as convenient or controllable. cremin (1981, in knowles, 1990) indicates that the revolution in learning that began in the twentieth century and is continuing into the twenty-first century may be as fundamental as the original invention of formalised learning institutions. a strong case for the adoption of alternative approaches to learning is made by capra (1982, in knowles, 1990). he argues that "we are trying to apply the concepts of an outdated world view the mechanistic world view of cartesian-newtonian science to a reality that can no longer be understood in these terms. we live in a globally interconnected world, in which biological, psychological, and environmental phenomena are all interdependent. to describe this world appropriately, an ecological perspective is needed that the cartesian world view cannot offer" (capra, 1982, in knowles, 1990: 19). capra further argues that a fundamental change is needed in thoughts, perceptions and values, and thus, attitudes. the beginnings of this change are visible in x most areas and are likeljj to result "in a transformation of unprecedented dimensions, a turning point for our planet as a whole" (capra, 1982-, in knowles: 19). thus, a paradigm shift is needed in order to create new learning systems that focus on the development of potential which is achieved through teaching how to learn and not what to learn. there are many reasons for a paradigm shift in learning, not the least of which is that in the usa more than 40% of school-going children are diagnosed as having some kind of learning problem (jensen, 1995; thornburg, 1991, in jensen, 1995; simon, 1987). however, it is felt that this percentage is in fact higher, closer to 90% than 40%, and that the reason for this high percentage is the system within which pupils are being "educated". the current educational system is producing "educational casualties" (simon, 1987), rather than innovative lifelong learners. this constitutes a major problem because learning is an ongoing process that crosses all walks of life, and the application of traditional behaviouristic and cognitive learning systems is not preparing children for life (knowles, 1990). according to mitchell (1986, in buzan & dixon, 1976), society needs a more extended view of what normal human potential is, implying that high achievers are the norm and not the exception. this involves a totally new and broader approach to the perception of learning, and, therefore, of educating and remediating. in summary, it appears that traditional approaches to education and therapy are based on the i.q., piagetian and classical information processing theories of learning. these approaches all focus on a certain kind of logical or' linguistic problem-solving; ignore neurobiology; do not deal with higher levels of creativity; and finally do not consider the ethnography of learning. the result of such approaches is less than optimal as, according to research, 7090% of students are underachieving , many of whom require additional support in the form of therapy (iran-nejad, 1990; bloom, 1984). bloom (1984, in iran-nejad, 1990) states that for more than thirty years students have been memorising facts and definitions without understanding them. sizer (1984:84) indicates that "students are all too often docile, compliant and without initiative, painting a picture of considerable passivity towards academic learning and school". a non-intelligent learning culture of not thinking has thus resulted, producing students who do not take responsibility for their learning, and who are reliant on external sources to do their thinking for them. the geodesic movement the geodesic movement by contrast, focuses on the symbolic vehicles of thought, namely the activities and products of the human mind such as language, mathematics, visual arts and gestures (gardner, 1985; irannejad, 1990: allport, 1980; hinton & anderson, 1981). the geodesic movement moves away from the search for general problem-solving devices and horizontal structures such as memory, attention and perception, and focuses more on vertical components, hence providing a more molar and molecular analysis of the nervous system (allport, 1980). this approach is not entirely new, as facets of the mind were already recognised in ancient greek philosophy, and it can thus be seen as a type of rejuvenated faculty psychology. the geodesic movement does not merely focus on the linguistic, logical and numerical symbols of piagetian, i.q., and information processing theories , but also focuses on a full range of symbol systems encompassing musical, bodily, spatial and personal symbol systems, and is consequently multimodal (gardner, 1985: leaf, 1997). each symbol system can be viewed as an independent functioning cluster of intelligences making up that particular symbolic domain, and, although separate, the domains do interact in the thinking process (gardner, 1985). the geodesic is biologically oriented and is based on brain organisation and maturational capacity. thinking is perceived to consist of a number of special purpose devices, or clusters of abilities presumably dependent on neural "hard-wiring" in the brain (allport, 1980). furthermore, cognitive accomplishments may occur in a range of domains some of which are universal, such as the logical-mathematical domain which forms the basis of experimentation in the i.q., piagetian and information processing movements (feldman, 1980). some are culturally specific such as reading which is important in some cultures and not in others. within each domain there are steps ranging from novice to expert, making the movement developmental. however, there are great inter-individual differences in the speed at which an individual passes through the stages from novice to masdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 tery in the different domains. in contrast to piaget's theory, success at negotiating one domain does not invoke the other domains (feldman, 1980). the development of domains is dependent on internal genetic factors as well as on external cultural factors (gardner, 1985). therefore the geodesic movement represents a shift from cognitive theories of knowledge to cognitive theories of how the nervous system functions. (iran-nejad, 1990; hinton & anderson, 1981). this development suggests that simultaneous learning in diverse local sites and subsystems of the nervous systems is the rule for learning, as opposed to one-thing-at-a-time rule of traditional approaches. according to iran-nejad (1990), if previously unrelated local sites in the brain, representing domains, are stimulated simultaneously in a brain-compatible way, they will combine in configurations previously not experienced and result in higher levels of functioning. this simultaneous learning hypothesis, which is central to the geodesic movement, suggests that more than active conscious control is needed to think, learn and release potential. it implies that another kind of control must be operating on the non-conscious level, and both types of control are needed in effective learning. this control is called dynamic self-regulation, and is simultaneous, implying that simultaneous functioning is a prerequisite for learning as opposed to the other way around as in traditional approaches. furthermore, dynamic self-regulation operates on the non-conscious level, yet will impact the conscious level through a process of attention delegation, which is the power to contribute to the learning process even though not conscious (iran-nejad, 1990). this simultaneous, non-conscious process permits individuals to engage in multimodal encoding, unencumbered by potential interference from one-modality-at-atime executive encoding which is characteristic of many learners exposed to traditional approaches, and results in a cautious literal attitude to learning. learning in the traditional mode becomes increasingly analytical, intentional and potentially very sequential because the learner is using the rehearsal-memorisation strategy of allocating immediate attention to every physical item of the task over and over again, without regard for the powerful contributions of spontaneous, tacit and explicit attention-delegation processes of dynamic internal self-regulation. the geodesic approach represents a way of creating a more thinking-orientated approach to learning. a fundamental tenet underlying geodesic learning is that a mediator cannot cause learning in an individual, learning must be created by the learner. thus the mediator should structure the environment to facilitate the learning process (feuerstein, 1980). within the alternative geodesic approach, with the altered perception of learning, is the implication of an expanded role for the speech-language therapist working in educational settings. traditionally the speech-language therapist has employed a clinical model of intervention focusing on the oral linguistic aspects of language * (simon, 1987; paul-brown, 1992). this latter approach has led to a focus on deficits and remediating deficits a symptomatic approach. for example, viewing syntax, semantics, pragmatics and auditory processing as separate variables while ignoring the reading and writing aspects of communication leads to fragmented services that drill splinter skills (simon, 1987; paul-brown, 1992). in the field of education, specifically simon (1987) postucaroline . leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw lates that well-meaning traditional speech-language approaches have actually ended up creating "educational casualties" as a consequence of segregating and labelling students, leading them to become addicted to 1:1 attention. this has led to the development of passive attitudes towards learning by falling into patterns of "learned helplessness" due to believing their "disabled" labels (johnson, 1987). alternative service delivery approaches have consequently emerged in response to the increasing awareness of the inefficiency of traditional approaches to communication, with immediate impact on the role of the speech-language therapist. thus, the most significant implication arising out of the literature related to the development of lifelong innovative learners with proficient communication skills, concerns the need to move from teaching and facilitating specific skills to the teaching of strategies to enable students to attain mature language repertoires and communication competence with adequate problem-solving skills (thornburg, 1991, in jensen, 1995; derry, 1990). the speech-language therapist, with a background in language, communication, psychology, speech and hearing science, linguistics, and learning theory, is eminently qualified to become involved in the integration of a geodesic approach to the process of learning and intellectual development, which indicates the expanded role for the speech-language therapist working with learning problems (paul-brown, 1992). as discussed, speech-language therapists should view themselves as language specialists concerned with the prevention and remediation of communication difficulties by focusing on the process of learning and intellectual development (thornburg, 1991, in jensen, 1995). the speech-language therapist is seen to play an important role academically in assisting with adapting the child's academic instruction so that he can achieve to the best of his ability (committee on language, speech and hearing services in schools, 1983, in johnson, 1987). this implies that a complex relationship exists between language used for learning and intellectual development and language used for communication, highlighting the need for a paradigm shift in the perception of learning. in view of the foregoing, research highlighting and emphasising the necessity of a learning paradigm change is needed if educational institutions are to facilitate the development of innovative lifelong learners that can make a contribution to society. ι the overall objective of the research on the geodesic information processing model, and its application, the mind-mapping approach (mma) (leaf, 1990; 1997), is therefore to create and explore an alternative system to the traditional learning system. the mind-mapping approach (mma), is believed to provide a better way to assist learners teachers, therapists, pupils and clients alike in becoming innovative lifelong learners. this is because the theoretical base of the mma incorporates the principles inherent in the philosophy of geodesic learning, which is the suggested alternative philosophy upon which the perception of learning should be based,, and which falls within the realms of a humanistic approach. the mma framework provides a basis for bridging the gap between the unique individual learner and the design and delivery of the learning experience. the latter is in contrast to traditional educational and institutional systems of facilitating learning and communication, which are based on unnatural behaviouristic, mechanistic and cogthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a model for geodesic learning: the geodesic information processing model 57 nitive philosophies which undermine the complexity and hence the potential of human nature. in view of the complex nature of learning, intellectual potential and communication, approaches of a more geodesic nature need to be adopted by educationalists and s p e e c h l a n g u a g e therapists in learning institutions in order to increase the efficacy of service delivery (simon, 1987). this implies a paradigm shift from traditional behaviouristic and cognitive approaches to geodesic approaches to learning and communication (gardner, 1985; dhority, 1991; jensen, 1995). this paradigm shift can be facilitated by practical frameworks incorporating geodesic principles. the mmais viewed as a framework of this nature. _ this theoretical model also serves as a foundation lor the expansion of the concept of the mind-map (leaf, 1990; leaf, uys & louw, 1993). the expanded view of mindmapping views the mind-map as the key which accesses the non-conscious levels of the brain. the mind-map is seen to directly access and influence the thought processes serving to unlock the potential of the brain. hence the mind-map will be shown to be the creative symbolic visualisation of the raw material of consciousness, that is, the synchronised electrical-chemical reactions of the neurons. it is the purpose of this paper to explain the development of the theoretical model, the geodesic information processing model in terms of its operation and theoretical assumptions. the importance of a theoretical base in the development and application of an alternative approach to facilitating a creative learning process in students and clients is stressed. the concepts inherent in the geodesic information processing model are general geodesic principles and can therefore be extrapolated and used in therapy and education . the mma and the expanded conception of the mind-map do, however, provide a framework that incorporates all geodesic principles in a practical way, and will be discussed in ensuing papers. explanation of the geodesic information processing model i in this section, the components of the geodesic information processing model and their operation will be described. orientation i 1 the geodesic information processing model was developed to explain the thinking process invoked by using the geodesic techniques of the mma. the emphasis of the mmais to capitalise on the natural multimodal functioning of the brain in order to reconceptualise useful knowledge and develop potential. the four components of a geodesic approach, namely metacognition, cognition, neuropsychology and symbolism, are incorporated into the geodesic model. the theoretical underpinnings of the development of the model have been derived from contemporary brain research: the work of iran-nejad (1990) on the two-source theory of self-regulation; lozanov's (1975, 1978) development of suggestopeadia; gardner's (1980, 1985) research on symbolic systems and the multiple intelligence theory; contemporary metacognitive and cognitive research, specifically on the role of the non-conscious (flavell, 1978); and finally symbolic system1 approaches to information processing which use the brain as the analogy for the mind (hinton & anderson, 1981). the geodesic information processing model is presented in schematic form in figure 1. this is accompanied by a gestalt overview of its operation followed by an in-depth explanation and discussion of its components and their operation. an overview of the operation of the geodesic information processing model the geodesic information processing model (figure 1) is a hypothetical model that traces the information processing pathway from the input which can be internal, external or both to the output whilst using a geodesic framework such as the mma. it is, however, postulated that this model can be extrapolated to explain any approach that strives to facilitate the processing of information within an environment that follows the natural laws of functioning of the brain. as the result of an internal or external input, or both, information begins to be processed (see figure 1). if the geodesic framework, the mma is utilised, specific metacognitive module(s) will be activated by a patternmatching process. this will result in the activation of the processing systems of the specific metacognitive module^) to be involved in the task. in order for the processing system to operate, metacognitive action (see figure 1), needs to be instituted, that is, the interaction of declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge executed by dynamic self-regulation. this will lead to the selection of the function to be carried out, facilitated by the activation of existing descriptive systems to assist in the reconceptualisation of the new knowledge, and the cognitive process will begin. at this point, active and dynamic self-regulation interact. the quality of this interaction is controlled by the geodesic nature of the mma. the cognitive domain (see figure 1), has to match the processing system already selected, and therefore the cognitive requirements of content, form and use will need to be met before quality processing can continue. if the cognitive requirements are fulfilled, then the cognitive function^) will be selected to carry out the cognitive task to completion. in order to operationalise the cognitive function^), cognitive action begins. finally, the result of the information processing will be expressed through a symbolic format which is known as the output. the evidence of the newly reconceptualised knowledge is visually available on the mind-map, and represents the overt evidence that thought has taken place. the geodesic information processing model (figure 1) is divided into four components, namely the metacognitive, cognitive, symbolic and neuropsychological components. the operation and interaction of each of these components is now discussed. the metacognitive component description the metacognitive component comprises seven metacognitive module's (see figure 1), each of which can be broken down into processing systems. each processing system is made up of functions that realise the potential die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 caroline . leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw the southafrican journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a model for geodesic learning: the geodesic information processing model knowledge. 59 of that processing system. this realisation of the potential of a processing system is made possible by its computational capacity, the activation of the metacognitive domain. the seven metacognitive modules the metacognitive modules represent the knowledge base of the mind, categorised into seven groups based on the multiple intelligence theory (gardner, 1981, 1985). these seven are not exhaustive but are seen to be representative of the range of human knowledge and intellectual potentials (gardner, 1985; kline, 1990). the seven modules are the linguistic, logical /mathematical, visual/spatial, musical, interand intrapersonal, and kinaesthetic domains of knowledge. according to gardner (1985), owing to heredity, early training or both, some individuals will experience greater development within some domains of knowledge than others, but every normal individual should develop each domain to some extent. in life, these domains of knowledge (called metacognitive modules in the current model because of the metacognitive and information processing perspective) work in harmony, and so their autonomy may be invisible. however, gazzaniga (1977, in gardner, 1985) argues that they function as independent units each with their own cognitive characteristics. it is therefore proposed that the integrative cognitive nature of the mma facilitates the interaction of these modules. when these modules interact, higher order thinking is produced because the net result of the interaction between modules improves the quality of interaction within modules. strength in the sum of the parts is the fundamental principle of this modular perspective. the quality of higher cortical functions is influenced by the harmonious interaction of modules which is facilitated by creating environments that tap the abilities of all the modules, as opposed to just one or two, as is the case with traditional approaches. synchronised interaction is facilitated within multimodal frameworks such as the mma. i it should be noted that in figure 1 (the geodesic information processing model), the expansion from the metacognitive level to the cognitive level, to the symbolic level is shown only in the linguistic metacognitive module. however, it is proposed that each metacognitive module follows this selfsame expansion within its domain of metacognitive knowledge component | mma | e x e c u t e r dynamic s e l f r e g u l a t i o n monitoring ] oper ates i proce sys ssin3 tem regulation goal s e t t i n g figure 2: the metacognitive domain the processing systems and their functions a metacognitive module is further subdivided into processing systems which (see figure 1) are the result of a whole system of functions. these are represented neurologically by interrelations of different parts of the brain, based on luria's (1980) conception of functional systems. each metacognitive module has its own specific processing systems, which are represented across both hemispheres in the brain. the processing systems are made up of functions which are locally represented in specific areas of either the left or right hemisphere of the brain. for example, the linguistic metacognitive module has various different processing systems such as reading, writing, communicating and listening. each of these, in turn, can be divided into their functions. thus, for the processing system of reading, the function could be reading to find the key concept, or reading a complex technical manual, or reading a novel for pleasure. each of these functions requires different cognitive approaches and is made up of various different steps termed cognitive actions which will operationalise the cognitive task. the metacognitive domain the computational capacity a computational capacity exists at the core of each metacognitive module which is unique to that particular metacognitive module, and on which its complex realisations are based. from the repeated use of, interaction among, and elaboration of the various computational devices, forms of knowledge will eventually flow that could be termed useful, thus contributing to intelligence. these forms of knowledge have the potential to be involved in symbol systems, and will ultimately be expressed on the symbolic level. more specifically, these computational capacities, which are unique to each of the metacognitive modules, are termed the metacognitive domains. a metacognitive domain comprises declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge with its executor being dynamic self-regulation. each processing system operates under the direction of the metacognitive domain for that particular metacognitive module. figure 2 illustrates this relationship schematically. an example of the interplay in the metacognitive domain a hypothetical example of the dynamic interplay between the declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge components, and their executor dynamic self-regulation as invoked by the mma would involve the following. initially an externallyor self-imposed goal is established (the equivalent of internal or external input). the existing metacognitive knowledge concerning this particular objective leads to the conscious metacognitive experience (interaction between active and dynamic self-regulation) that the objective may be difficult to achieve. this metacognitive self-regulation, combined with additional metacognitive knowledge, results in the selection and use of the cognitive strategy (termed cognitive act on the schema) of asking quesdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 caroline . leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw tions of oneself or knowledgeable other people. the answers arising from this exercise stimulate additional metacognitive experiences about the success of the task, which represents the interaction between active and dynamic self-regulation. these experiences, guided by relevant metacognitive knowledge (declarative, procedural and conditional), investigate the cognitive strategy (cognitive act) of surveying, to establish whether it forms a coherent whole which provides a solution to the problem. this overview may result in the identification of difficulties with consequent activation by metacognitive knowledge and experience (active and dynamic self-regulation) of the same or different cognitive strategies. this interplay continues until the symbolic representation is achieved, which is the final creation of the mind-map. the activation of the metacognitive module metacognitive action in order to activate a metacognitive module, the components of the metacognitive domain (declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge) need to interact. this interaction results in metacognitive action, and is orchestrated by dynamic self-regulation. the quality of the interaction of the metacognitive domain determines the eventual output. in the creation of the mind-map, all three types of knowledge need to be considered when selecting the concepts, as well as when representing these in an associated way. the process of creating the mind-map enhances the interaction of declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge, resulting in metacognitive action. when metacognitive action occurs, the process of cognition begins. as mentioned earlier, 90 per cent of learning takes place on the non-conscious level (gardner, 1985; iran-nejad, 1990). the rationale for this is that "intelligent" learning is creative and multisource, and hence takes place on the non-conscious level (iran-nejad, 1990). traditional approaches assume that learning occurs under active conscious executive control, namely from a single source termed metacognition (iran-nejad, 1990; dhority, 1991). as a result effortful attention (iran-nejad, 1990) is viewed as the single most important regulator of learning (bereiter, 1985). this limits the domain of learning. in the current study, based on a literature review on the non-conscious and self-regulation, metacognition is redefined as occurring on the non-conscious level (irannejad, 1990; dhority, 1991; lozanov, 1978; flavell, 1978). these authors postulate the notion that external and internal stimuli are far too complex to manage or hold with only the mechanisms of our conscious attention. hence the non-conscious level is not viewed as simply containing the unattended or unimportant percepts, but as the level where the complex mental activity occurs. a structure for the non-conscious level is postulated, thus providing a broader definition of metacognition. by implication, 90 p.er cent of learning is taking place when metacognitive action is in process, and hence this is the level that should be targeted in intervention and mediation. the activation of the non-conscious stores triggers metacognitive action. the mma can be seen as this trigger. by implication, the mma focuses at the root level of the learning process, and therefore predominantly on the non-conscious. conversely, the cognitive functions that have been activated by metacognitive action, and that are orchestrated by active and dynamic self-regulation, only represent approximately 10 per cent of the learning and reconceptualisation of knowledge process (reddy, 1979, in iran-nejad, 1990; lozanov, 1975). the neurobiological level of metacognitive action on a biological level, modular columns of neuronal cells ascending from the cortex to the subcortex to the limbic system across the left and right hemispheres, represent the metacognitive modules and functional systems (see figure 1) (feldman, 1980). metacognitive action is represented as the distributed parallel activation of dendritic interconnections from the cortex to the limbic system across both hemispheres. the neuropsychological level of metacognitive action pattern recognition on a neuropsychological level, metacognitive action can be perceived as the activation of the descriptive systems (goldberg & costa, 1981), or organisational codes. the number of descriptive systems activated is dependent on the complexity of the cognitive task. these will be used to reconceptualise new descriptive systems based on pattern recognition. the mind-map's pattern structure facilitates the pattern recognition process, as well as making it available to introspection, and in this way more efficient use of the descriptive systems can be made. the interaction of active and dynamic self-regulation active self-regulation occurs on a conscious level, which implies that conscious introspection can occur. this is the result of the interaction of dynamic and active selfregulation (see hypothetical example above). according to iran-nejad (1990), this interaction has important implications for learning because the quality of this interaction distinguishes between effective and ineffective approaches to learning. therefore, active and dynamic self-regiilation have to interact in order to produce cognition. once the cognitive process is instituted, active and dynamic self-regulation should continue to interact, and the nonconscious will impact on the conscious level through attention delegation. this will lead to "quality learning". however, if active self-regulation starts to operate at the expense of dynamic self-regulation, which can occur if the facilitation is brain-antagonistic (jensen, 1995), as in traditional approaches, then the quality of learning lessens and learning becomes more rote-like. a single-source theory of self-regulation implies that the central executive must monitor constructive change by directly allocating attention to the source of change (reddy, 1979, in iran-nejad, 1990). the two-source alternative (iran-nejad, 1990) implies that active allocation of attention is neither sufficient nor always necessary. an activated descriptive system (the result of metacognitive action) can influence a cognitive task, even though it may be outside conscious awareness. therefore the activated descriptive system will still influence higher mental functions or cognitive tasks and strategies being used to complete a goal be it of a communicative or academic nature. this is known as attention-delegation power, which is the the southafrican journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) development of a model for geodesic learning: the geodesic information processing model 61 power to continue an ongoing contribution to internal r e c o n s t r u c t i o n even after t h e executive spotlight moves on to another site (iran-nejad, 1990). the most direct source of interaction between active and dynamic self-regulation occurs when specific attention is allocated to the components (iran-nejad, 1990). these are the declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge components of the metacognitive domain in the information process model. it is postulated that this interaction is enhanced by the mma due to its metacognitive nature. this process is evidenced during the act of creating a mind-map, where essentially declarative, procedural and conditional knowledge is stored. in the selection of a concept, metacognitive action sequences are established which indicate the associative relationships in a deductive and inductive way, therefore analogically. thinking on this level is considered to be deep processing as the metacognitive level is actively involved. if the incorrect concept is selected due to lack of comprehension or the attempt to learn in a rote fashion, incorrect action sequences will be stored, which will affect recall. this is easily rectified by reviewing the networked patterned nature of the mind-map. in this way the metacognitive components (which have become conscious by their visual symbolic expression on the mind-map, and therefore regulated by active self-regulation) will be activated. however, the metacognitive action sequences are governed by the non-conscious level (anderson, 1986, in springer & deutsch, 1989), and therefore dynamic selfregulation, and will be activated on a non-conscious level. hence, in order to rectify the incorrect action sequences, active and dynamic self-regulation have to meet and interact. it is this interaction that becomes a primary focus of the mma because, as already stipulated, the quality of interaction will distinguish between effective and ineffective approaches to learning. it is hypothesised that the mma improves' the quality of the interaction because it accesses the cognitive and metacognitive levels in its construction. figure 3 illustrates the relationship between active and dynamic self-regulation and metacognition and cognition. therefore, the metacognitive nonconscious is the highest level of thought, where qualitative, intelligent and useful knowledge is reconceptualised. traditional forms of stimulation will result in only partial activation of this level, arid hence the lack of activation of potential. j the cognitive component description the cognitive component of the geodesic information processing model represents what is traditionally assumed to be metacognition, that is, "thinking about thinking" (flavell, 1978). in the model (see figure 1), this level represents the level on which slow, conscious control of the thought process occurs. it is under the control of the central executive and is inherently sequential. the cognitive process begins after metacognitive action is instituted, when dynamic and active self-regulation interact. the cognitive process when metacognitive action is instituted, cognition (see figure 1), orchestrated by active and dynamic self-regulation, begins, the interaction between the two being of paramount importance. on the cognitive level, the metacognitive action is carried out to completion. this completive action is constantly enhanced by the use of the mma framework. in order to complete the metacognitive action, the cognitive process will be instituted on the product of the metacognitive action. this is the process that is instituted as the mind-map is being made. these action sequence steps include the following: • attention allocation and delegation; • perception through all the sensory modalities; • the decoding (analysis) of the incoming information (this involves the analysis of existing appropriate descriptive systems already called up when metacognitive action began) in preparation of the new reconceptualisation of knowledge; • the process of problem-solving, which includes reasoning, both deductive and inductive, resulting in inferences and judgements being made, and cause-effect relationships being established; • the organisation of the resultant reconceptualisations into appropriately associated and categorised networks; and finally • the synthesising (encoding) of information that will be effectively stored in memory (flavell, 1978; gardner, 1985; iran-nejad, 1990; jensen, 1995; dhority, 1991; hart, 1983; hand, 1986). these cognitive processes occur within each of the cognitive domains which delineate the processing systems already activated on the metacognitive level, namely listening, speaking, reading and writing, in the case of the linguistic module. each cognitive domain has various requirements that have to be fulfilled in order to create useful knowledge. these requirements (bloom & lahey, 1978, in leaf, 1997) include: somemetacognition (non-conscious) point at which active • dynamic vself-regulatiori cognition (conscious) rtwe • figure 3: the interaction between active and dynamic self-regulation die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 caroline . leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw thing to communicate (content); a structure for the communication (form); and, finally, a communicative intent (use). the next phase of the mma information processing model proposes that the cognitive process operates within each cognitive domain selected for the specific task at hand, if the cognitive requirements of useful knowledge have been fulfilled. each cognitive domain is divided into cognitive functions, corresponding to the functions on the metacognitive level, which are further subdivided into cognitive actions. in this way the cognitive act operationalises the cognitive task. an example of cognition in action level is therefore the evidence that thinking and processing of information has occurred. this is expressed through a symbolic vehicle that is representative of the metacognitive module. for example, the linguistic metacognitive module can be expressed symbolically as oral expression, reception, written expression or reading, or all of these (see figure 2.1). the mind-map facilitates and represents all four forms of expression. from the symbolic level, judgements of a person's thinking, learning, intellectual potential and communication skills are made. this implies judgements as to the effectiveness of cognitive and metacognitive skills. this occurs because metacognition influences cognition which in turn influences the symbolic output. in selecting the cognitive domain (see figure 1) (processing system) of writing, the cognitive function may be to write down a selected concept onto the mind-map. this is cf1 (cognitive function 1) in figure 1. cf1 would then be made up of various cognitive acts (ca1, ca2 etc) which are the steps involved in carrying out cf1. thus, ca1 in this case would be the analysis of the phonemes that would afterwards have to be written. this involves the posterior parts of the left temporal zone (luria, 1978). ca2 would involve the motoric expression of the lingual sound (luria, 1980) in order to make the contents of the sound clear. this involves the inferior portion of the left post-central gyrus (luria, in leaf, 1990). ca3 is the transferring of phonemes into letters involving the spatial arrangement of the graphemes, which involves the parietal-occipital part of the cortex (luria, 1980). ca4 involves the sequencing of phonemes and graphemes while writing, which involves the pre-motor zone (luria, 1980). finally, ca5 will involve the positioning of the word on the mind-map to fit into the associative network. thus ca5, in this case, moves onto the symbolic level namely level three on the information processing model. the symbolic component the symbolic component comprises the expressive level of the cognitive action, which is in turn influenced by the metacognitive component. functioning on the symbolic figure 4: a schematic representation of the neurobiological arrangements of the metacognitive modules the neuropsychological component the last component of the geodesic information processing model is the neuropsychological component, which deals with the relationship between brain function and behaviour (see figure 1) (tollman, 1988, in leaf, 1990). this component is the link between the biological and cognitive levels. in order to fall within the realms of being geodesic, the brain-function-behaviour relationship cannot be overlooked (dhority, 1991). according to the model, the metacognitive modules are represented biologically as modular columns of neuronal cells ascending from the cortex to the limbic system across both left and right hemispheres. it is postulated that there are seven neuronal columns representing the seven metacognitive domains, as illustrated in figure 4. as the result of input, electrical activity will flow across the columns. the more synergistic the input, the more synchronised the flow between the two hemispheres. it is postulated that when this occurs, larger areas of the brain will be utilised more efficiently. it is believed that this synchronised synergistic flow will result in the metacognitive action being activated. thus, the reserve capacities will be mobilised. in contrast, input from traditional approaches will result in reduced unsynchronised flow between the hemispheres resulting in only active self-regulation and effortful cognition occurring. the metacognitive domain is represented biologically as the distributed parallel activation of dendritic interconnections and synapses within the neuronal columns of the modules across both hemispheres (cook, 1984, in springer & deutsch, 1989). neuropsychological^, this results in pattern detection (pribram, 1971, in leaf,1997; hart, 1983, in leaf, 1997), which is the calling up of existing descriptive systems to facilitate reconceptualisation of knowledge. the cognitive component is represented as localised activation of neural connections in either the left or right hemisphere, because the processing systems at this'' stage are more specific (springer & deutsch, 1989). finally the symbolic component is represented as parallel activation of the modules involved across both hemispheres because the symbolic expression is the result of synery the southafrican journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a model for geodesic learning: eristic action. „ , , therefore the geodesic information processing model rovides speculation as to the type of thinking that is induced when working within a geodesic framework. it traces the processing of information from the metacognitive level through to the symbolic output. there are various assumptions upon which this model is based. a n e x a m i n a t i o n of these assumptions and their theoretical underpinnings is of relevance, as they lead to a redefinition of the non-conscious level, metacognition, cognition and learning. these redefinitions are pivotal in the explanation of the effectiveness of a geodesic approach to intervention and education. in the ensuing discussion, an overview of the eight assumptions of the model is presented. this is followed by a more detailed explanation. the eight assumptions of the geodesic model: an overview there are eight assumptions underlying the geodesic information processing model (see figure 1 and table 1). the first of these assumptions deals with the metacognitive component of the model. it is assumed that metacogthe geodesic information processing model 63 nition is the root of the thought process controlling the cognitive process and ultimately the symbolic output. furthermore, the key to unlocking intellectual potential occurs when the metacognitive level is activated effectively. it is postulated that traditional approaches, which are not geodesic, do not take full advantage of the metacognitive potential of the brain, and that the full spectrum of metacognition is thus overlooked. within a geodesic approach such as the mma, it is assumed that the metacognitive level is more adequately activated. the second assumption postulates that metacognition is the non-conscious level. this implies that the majority of complex higher cortical functioning and learning occurs outside conscious awareness (reddy, 1979, in iran-nejad, 1990; derry, 1990). the way that metacognition is conceptualised within the geodesic information processing model provides a structure for understanding and analysing the non-conscious level. the third assumption deals with the concept of selfregulation and relates to the metacognitive and cognitive components of the geodesic information processing model. traditionally, this is the conscious executive control of thought which forms part of the definition of metacognition (costa, 1984). according to slife, weiss and bell table 1: the assumptions and theoretical underpinnings of the geodesic information processing model level assumptions theoretical underpinnings metacognition 1. metacognition is the non-conscious level that accounts for the bulk of learning 2. the metacognitive structure of the non-conscious: (1)metacognitive modules (2) metacognitive processing systems (3) metacognitive domains 3. the interaction of active and dynamic self-regulation is the operating system of effective thought processing. automaticity research multi-source self-regulation theory modular theory suggestopaedia multiple intelligence theory lurian theory metacognitive research descriptive system theory 4. the cognitive component is the level on which conscious sequential thought occurs. 5. memory enhancement, as part of the cognitive process, is contextual and content based specific to each module. self-regulation theory cognitive research taxon and local memory memory enhancement research ! i 6.1 synergy between the hemisphers releases potential. 6.2 metacognitive results in the activation of descriptive systems through the process of pattern recognition and feedback creating open systems. 6.3 the brain is a modular system of interlinked functional systems. 6.4 the limbic system needs to be activated in order to reconceptualize useful knowledge. 6.5 the processing of information occurs in a parallel simultaneous fashion on a nonconscious level, and in a sequential way on a conscious level. hemisphericity research topographic inhibition theory descriptive systems pattern recognition feedback modularity theory cognitive-emotive theory suggestopaedia pdp theory modular theory 7. the capacity to express and communicate using some symbolic vehicle. symbolic system modular theory 1 8. intelligent learning is the reconceptualization of descriptive systems leading to new knowledge. self-regulation theory suggestopaedia die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 caroline . leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw (1985 in iran-nejad, 1990), self-regulation refers to the planning, monitoring and checking activities necessary to orchestrate cognition. iran-nejad (1990) however, argues that this type of self-regulation, termed active self-regulation, is only part of the self-regulation process, accounting for the learning of a functional knowledge base. overreliance on this active self-regulation results in rote learners and reduces learning potential (iran-nejad, 1990). it is proposed that an additional form of self-regulation, termed dynamic self-regulation, is required to overcome the inherent limitations of active self-regulation (iran-nejad, 1990). dynamic self-regulation is rapid, spontaneous, multimodal and co-ordinates the simultaneous as opposed to the sequential aspects of the learning process (iran-nejad, 1990). it is the interaction of these two types of self-regulation that will lead to more effective learning (iran-nejad, 1990, 1991). within the geodesic information processing model, the interaction of the two types of self-regulation is viewed as the operating system of effective thought processing. this interaction triggers metacognitive action (see figure 1). the fourth assumption deals with the cognitive component, which is responsible for the conscious sequential aspect of learning. the activation of the cognitive process is reliant on its interaction with metacognition. this in turn is orchestrated by the interaction of active and dynamic self-regulation. therefore, according to the geodesic information processing model, the conscious awareness, or the "thinking about thinking" aspect of the thought process is a more advanced level of cognition and not metacognition, as described in traditional definitions. the fifth assumption, also dealing with the cognitive component, is that memory enhancement is part of the cognitive process. therefore, although memory is stored on the non-conscious metacognitive level, the actual enhancing of the memory process is facilitated by various techniques that are consciously created on the cognitive level and expressed on the symbolic level. the sixth assumption of the geodesic information processing model is concerned with the neuropsychological component (see figure 1). research has indicated that the most effective way of releasing the potential of the brain is through stimulating a synergistic wholistic and complementary pattern of processing between the two hemispheres (springer & deutsch, 1989). this will allow the natural, wholistic pattern-discrimination ability of the brain to function. priibram (1971, in leaf, 1997) argues that the brain extracts meaning through wholistic multisource pattern discrimination rather than through single facts or lists. the human brain is not designed for linear unimodal thought, but operates by simultaneously going down many paths (hart, 1983, in leaf, 1997). hart (1983) stresses the importance of presenting and assimilating information in larger patterns before the details. thus, a geodesic framework will need to utilise formats of presenting and assimilating information that allow synergistic multimodal pattern discrimination to occur. the techniques of the mma, specifically the mind-map, are assumed to stimulate multisource pattern discrimination that is brain-compatible (leaf, 1990; leaf et al., 1993). the seventh assumption of the geodesic information processing model deals with the symbolic component (see figure 1). the symbolic component is the expression of the metacognitive action, which is operationalised through the cognitive process. the symbolic component deals with the capacity of human beings to express and communicate meanings through using some symbolic vehicle (allport, 1980). it is assumed that the symbolic component reflects the thought processing of the person, and is the medium through which the thought process can be manipulated. the eighth assumption, relating to all four components of the geodesic information processing model, indicates that intelligent learning is the result of the reconceptualisation of knowledge (iran-nejad, 1990). the reconceptualisation of knowledge is the end result of the thought process invoked by a geodesic framework such as the mma. this is in contrast to traditional perceptions of learning which view learning as the incremental internalisation of external knowledge (reddy, 1979, in irannejad, 1990; costa, 1984). this latter definition cannot account for the complex creative process involved in intelligent learning and limits learning to the development of a factual knowledge base. implications of the geodesic information processing model arising out of the ideas presented in the current paper, are various implications for the speech-language therapist. these include the following :learning is the reconceptualisation of knowledge as opposed to the internal incrementalisation of facts. traditional philosophy limits the domain of learning to the simplistic internalization of externally available knowledge resulting in predominantly rote-type learning of facts and definitions (iran-nejad, 1990). most of the factual information taught within traditional environments has questionable value in terms of lifeskills, and therefore lacks in quality and usefulness (glasser, 1986). learning is an interactive multimodal process system, not a sequential accrual system which is only a sub-function. it is thus limiting and inhibiting to design education and therapeutic intervention around simple behaviouristic one-thing-at-a-time stimulus response. this is not congruent with biological theories of brain functioning, which indicate that the genetic structure of the brain results in behaviour being the attempt to satisfy needs, and is thus proactive, not reactive and stimulus bound (glasser, 1986). if the alternative perception of geodesic learning discussed in this paper is adopted, then the emphasis will move from the memorising of facts, information and formulas, which are readily available in both books and computer software, to processes and skills. for instance, preferred activities would be writing a play, as opposed to a grammatical writing lesson; or using co-operative groups to solve problems, or to understand a process as opposed to learning photostatted notes off by heart for a test. the idea is to immerse learners in multimodal stimulation using as many varied learning opportunities as possible. the focus would then be on the process of how to learn, which is recreating knowledge, and would therefore avoid simple incrementalisation of existing facts. the reconceptualisation of knowledge would enable students and clients to develop their ability to use what is learned, not just to know what it is. a further consideration is that special'education environments dealing with children with learning difficulties are usually characterised by a passive-acceptant approach the southafrican journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) development of a model for geodesic learning: the geodesic information processing model (feuerstein, 1980). familiar simple subject matter is offered at a slower pace with emphasis on reproducing material a simple incrementalisation of facts (feuerstein, 1980; king & goodman, 1990). this results in learning environments lacking any creativity, the facilitation of higher levels of thinking or the independent performance of higher level functions the reconceptualisation of knowledge. in order to raise children with learning difficulties to higher levels of development, a passiveacceptant approach must be replaced by a proactive geodesic approach. according to this approach, the individual is an open system capable of mental and emotional modification (feuerstein, 1980; jensen, 1995). a proactive geodesic approach will encourage children with learning difficulties to be actively involved in normal education environments and society. this requires a process of integration facilitated by the professional in the learning environment, who needs to recognise that language, learning and communication do not exist in a vacuum (paulbrown, 1992; marvin, 1987). instruction to facilitate language, learning and communication skills should be presented in natural environments requiring communication (johnson, 1987). as school is normally the natural environment for most school-going children, communication skills need to be integrated with academic content. thus, if the professional, specifically the speech-language therapist who has expertise in language and communication, works directly in the classroom where the problems occur, strategies can be provided for pupils to better understand academic material and classroom instructions (paulbrown, 1992). viewed functionally, speech-language therapists are not "re-mediating" or "re-habilitating" communication, language and learning disabilities, but are attempting to proactively assist in the mediation of a school communication system for them. in this way, the passive reactive incrementalisation of existing facts can be replaced by the active recreation of knowledge. learning environments need to be ecologically congruent and authentic with; an emic perspective, in order to facilitate effective language, learning and communication the majority of learning needs to be contextually embedded as realistically as is possible (johnson, 1987). this is because "the brain is actually very poor at learning large amounts of material from books. it is naturally good at learning in the locations and circumstances of everyday life" (jensen, 1995: 333). knowledge is more easily reconceptualised into useful! knowledge that can be utilised when it is associated with a,novel experience, or location or feeling, or some type of hook that will tie it in with the content. therefore neither the traditional "stand and deliver" context of teaching nor the isolated 1:1 therapy model are authentic, ecologically congruent or emic as the focus is on the teacher and therapist delivering content or remediating an identified "deficit". rather, the learner needs to be guided to discover the meaning of the content. furthermore, language, learning and communication are active creative processes. whether the focus is on speaking, listening, reading, or writing, language and communication involve the creation of meaning and making sense (king & goodman, 1990). a curriculum or therapeutic approach that fragments language, communication and learning into small, abstract pieces with the expectation that if the parts are mastered, the whole will eventually be mastered, inhibits learning and communication (schory, 1990). the opposite perspective is a whole language perspective where the learning direction is from the whole to the parts, (king & goodman, 1990; schory, 1990), and therefore falls within the realms of a geodesic approach. according to schory (1990), children pass a crucial test, before school, suggesting that they are spontaneously proficient learners, because they master in a few years one of the most complex systems of rules known, their mother tongue. they also become quite proficient in the knowledge of the world around them (iran-nejad, 1990). by contrast, "only a few children in school ever become good at learning in the way we try to make them learn. most of them get humiliated, frightened and discouraged. they use their mind, not to learn, but to get out of the things we tell them to do to make them learn" (holt, 1964: vii). bereiter (1985, in iran-nejad, 1990) indicates that there is a complex relationship between the multisource nature of learning and the environment in which this learning is fostered. a young child's learning environment is multisource, creative and natural with the various sources that contribute, operating simultaneously. it facilitates a balance between active and dynamic self-regulation to occur. this is in contrast to the less than authentic traditional learning environments of later life that foster a climate of encoding facts in an increasingly analytic and sequential way. this fosters an over-reliance on untrained or incorrectly-trained active self-regulation at the expense of dynamic self-regulation, which results in training children out of the natural way of learning (holt, 1964, in iran-nejad, 1990). therefore, the more wholistic, natural and meaningful the learning environment, is the more ecologically congruent and authentic it will be. this will ultimately result in more effective language, learning and communication skills. educationalists and therapists have a responsibility to change learning environments such that predominantly dynamic self-regulation operates with active self-regulation playing a minor role (iran-nejad, 1990; king & goodman, 1990; schory, 1990). this can be done by applying the principles of the philosophy of geodesic learning which have authentic ecological environments built into their methodology. geodesic approaches have to have authentic learning environments in order to work. by adopting geodesic approaches such as the mma, the practical application of the geodesic model, authentic learning environments will automatically be created. further research is needed to explore geodesic learning environments that foster a climate of authentic learning. in summary, transformation of learning in the in the schools of the future will need to consider the neuropsychological aspects that allow the interaction of dynamic and active self-regulation which will facilitate innovative learning. learning is a process of active research initiated and controlled by the learner. learning as a process of active research means that one's learning intentions need to be changed from those aimed at optimizing the conditions for encoding and retrieval under other-regulation to optimising the conditions for understanding and personal growth under selfregulation (iran-nejad, 1990). the latter implies that cooperation between teachers, therapists and pupils is required in the development of any course or therapy purporting to meet their needs. thus the learners take die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 caroline . leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw responsibility for their learning and the quality of their work in co-operation with the facilitator (glasser, 1986). this is in contrast to traditional philosophy of education and institution-based rehabilitation (ibr) which identifies the teacher and therapist as being solely responsible for what is learned, and how, when, why and if it is learned. currently, however, in the field of speech-language pathology, there is a move away from institution-based rehabilitation to community-based rehabilitation as a result of the increasing awareness of the inefficiency of traditional approaches, and the recognition of the social interactive nature of language (paul-brown, 1992). this is evident in the whole body of literature on alternative service delivery models designed to meet more efficiently the needs of clients with communication, language and learning problems (paul-brown, 1992; lewis, 1994; schory, 1990; king & goodman, 1992). if learners are guided into taking responsibility for their learning, then this will lead to learning that is based on curiosity, need and relevance, and thus the motivation becomes intrinsic. hence, the classroom and therapy room becomes focused on learning and not maintaining control. teachers and therapists have authority, and are content specialists, but learners also have the right to be respected and given an opportunity to learn. according to jensen (1995) and iran-nejad (1990), students in a fullyimplemented geodesic learning environment will rarely have behavioural, motivational and learning problems because they are fully engaged, curious, engrossed, challenged and excited about learning. students need to play a major role in the decision about what they have to learn and how this can be done; that is, learners need to take a higher level of responsibility for their own learning (schory, 1990; king & goodman, 1990). thus the learner needs to self-monitor and self-evaluate with the facilitator, with the emphasis on teaching the student how to assess the process and not just the end result of the process, the product (glasser, 1986). the learners and facilitators should engage in continual constructive examination of how to improve the process of learning. this can be done by the teacher and/or speech-language therapist orally making decisions and solving problems concerning her own reading, writing, communication or learning activity in order to demonstrate the problem-solving process (schory, 1990). this is especially important for the language-learning disabled students who frequently experience difficulty solving problems related to language, learning and communication (damico, 1987). finally, comparative studies should be conducted between self-regulated students in geodesic environments and other-regulated students in traditional environments in terms of problem-solving, research skills, thinking skills and general life skills in order to compare the differences in performance and learning potentials. there is research of this nature in the literature, but concerning predominantly suggestopaedic techniques (dhority, 1990; lozanov, 1978). there are, however, relatively few programmes that offer the unique combination of the mma, and it is felt that geodesic methods need to be used as wholistically as possible within a system's theory approach as opposed to componentially within a traditional approach to education. thus, true geodesic systems need to be created and studied scientifically in order to create the body of evidence that is lacking in traditional learning approaches. furthermore, this body of evidence will underscore the pitfalls of the traditional environments in education and therapy that were created, with relatively minimal scientific basis, (gardner, 1985: jensen, 1995; iran-nejad, 1990; knowles, 1990), as well as supporting the intimation made by gerber (1987) that traditional environments "de-educate" students turning them into rote-learning "junkies". in addition, the speculation that behavioural, motivational and learning problems will decrease in fully operating geodesic systems (jensen, 1995; iran-nejad, 1990) needs scientific and documented research as this has profound implications for students. this is because the ability to take a proactive role in initiating and controlling the learning process allows personal effort and ability to take on a determining role. according to glasser, (1986) persons who see themselves in control of a given situation make a greater effort to achieve success then those who do not. language-learning disabled pupils in particular need to be allowed to have a sense of control over their own learning processes in order to overcome the passiveacceptant and learned helplessness that comes from repeated failure and being continually guided (feuerstein, 1980). when a teacher or therapist continually corrects and guides students' efforts, they prevent them from taking charge of their own learning. this leads to overdependency on others and decreased confidence in one's own abilities (marvin, 1987). thus, the languagelearning disabled child needs to be shown how and allowed to take control of the language, learning and communication situation in and out of school. intelligence is pluralistic and in every individual there is a unique blend that determines their individuality. the multiple intelligence theory (gardner, 1985) challenges the prevailing concept of intelligence as a single general capacity that enables individuals to perform in all situations. according to gardner (1985) every normal human being is born with seven different intelligences. of these, one will be dominant and one secondary and this contributes to individualistic learning styles. if this does not conform to the dominant traditional teaching style, which emphasises verbal and mathematical intelligence, then individuals are at a disadvantage. thus, learning environments and facilitators need to recognise that intelligence is made up of different capacities, not just mathematical and linguistic, which results in a diversity of learning styles requiring highly individualised programmes and consequently, "freedom within structure". furthermore, i.q. testing, which is based on the single unitary concept of intelligence, cannot predict or determine potential as these tests are based on mathematical and linguistic intelligences alone. i.q. testing can only predict how well a student can play the "school game", and may erroneously label a student, limiting aspirations. successful teaching and therapy need to reinforce and affirm the different ways in which individuals learn. facilitators of learning need to incorporate situations where students have opportunities for the creative exploration of their individual interests and talents while also learning valued skills and concepts through multimodal means. information needs to be presented in numerous ways offering students many opportunities to succeed. therefore, manipulation and actual experience, moving, the southafrican journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) development of a model for geodesic learning: the geodesic information processing model 67 the hing and doing should be part of the learning process, τ rning environments need to help students to identify h i r areas of strength and to develop these so that they η become active contributors in society in their future. the more authentic the environment, the more effective t h e generalisation of skill mastery and problem-solving erformance will be. in addition, learning is more effective if a process (for example: learning plan) and opennded product structure (for example: therapy objectives, course outline) is applied, as opposed to a close-ended (traditional) product structure alone. within the field of s p e e c h l a n g u a g e pathology, the whole-language approach (schory, 1990) is evidence of this idea being practiced. the suggested geodesic methodology provides a broader framework enabling neuropsychological concepts to be incorporated into the whole-language approach, enhancing its effectiveness. the practical application of facilitating the seven different intelligences in learning environments is currently available in the literature (campbell et al., 1992, in leaf 1997). what is needed is scientific research incorporating these applications into geodesic frameworks such as the mma, and into learning environments in order to demonstrate their success. the learning approach needs to be transdisciplinary requiring facilitators, directors and therapists to assume interchangeable roles and responsibilities following the needs of the child, the family and the community a geodesic approach requires pupils, therapists, teachers and parents to commit to teaching and learning from each other by working together. this approach involves a collaborative and consultative methodology and as such, can be considered transdisciplinary (thurman & widerstrom, 1990). a transdisciplinary approach falls within the realms of systems theory which allows any social system to be conceptualised as a system of learning resources, or an interdependent learning community (knowles, 1990). a wholistic learning syst'em is a complex of elements in mutual interaction. | therefore, to account for wholism and interdependence, there has to be co-operative interaction between all the people within the system. the key issue, however, is the interchangeability of roles required and hence a transdisciplinary as opposed to interdisciplinary approach is essential for a truly geodesic learning environment to be created. in order to operationalise the above implication, collaborative and consultative skills have to be included in any training of teachers and therapists (all types) (simon, 1987). this would also include systems theory training which emphasises community-based learning systems. future research needs to explore the benefits of transdisciplinary principles within wholistic geodesic learning environments such as those created when using the mma, specifically the advantages of such an approach to the community as a whole. resources are readily available in every environment, and thus a primary research focus is to identify these and introduce learners to them. systems need to be put into effect where all resources within a community are explored and utilised in an organised interactive way within a geodesic framework. it is now recognised that services are most successful when teams of professionals and families collaborate forming partnerships. a transdisciplinary approach involves a collaborative consultative methodology involving both professionals and the community. teachers and therapists play different roles in a geodesic as opposed to traditional learning environments historically, the classroom teacher provided the student with the curriculum material to be learned, and the speech-language therapist provided the student with remediation strategies for specified communication difficulties (simon, 1987). however, the most important objective of a geodesic model such as the mma is adapting the child's academic instruction so that he can achieve to the best of his ability. many students are not successful learners and the differences between the educational experiences of students from different racial, linguistic and socio-economic backgrounds has led to many revisionist movements, which fall within the realms of geodesic philosophy, and which share the common goal of changing what does not appear to work. one direct result of this change is the re-discovery of the role language-proficiency plays in the education process (simon, 1987). here, the speech-language therapist, who is a language expert, can be extremely effective in mainstreaming into the classroom. this implies changed roles for both the speech-language therapist and the teacher who would need to work together in a consultative and collaborative manner in order to take advantage of their combined expertise. this whole-language approach (schory, 1990) would change the focus from the identification and fixing of deficits to the purpose and nature of learning. a teacher or therapist in a geodesic learning environment is a facilitator of learning. this implies that teachers and therapists are managers of the process of learning as opposed to content-transmitters. being a content resource or a content specialist should be a secondary role to that of being a facilitator of learning. according to knowles (1990) and glasser (1986), being a process manager as opposed to a content planner and transmitter requires relationship building, needs assessment, involvement of students in curricular planning, linking students to learning resources and encouraging student initiative. this idea is developed within the whole-language approach (schory, 1990; king & goodman, 1990) which provides a distinct philosophy as well as practical ideas on how to implement glasser (1986) and knowles' (1990) postulations. in order to operationalise the different roles of the teacher and therapist within the geodesic environment, classroom and curriculum-based models which utilise the concepts of collaboration and consultation have to be developed. classroom-based language and communication intervention has the distinct advantage of allowing the speech-language therapist to use the pupils' academic programmes as the basis upon which to build language intervention because pupils can stay in their classrooms and thus be present when important content information is given (schory, 1990). under such a system, known as the whole-language approach (schory, 1990), the speechlanguage therapist would be able to monitor the development of oral language skills within a more natural setting than a therapy room; there could be a more frequent exchange of information between the teacher and speechlanguage therapist regarding the specific needs of each language-learning disabled child resulting in improved die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 caroline . leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw language-learning experiences; there would also be the opportunity to provide teachers with suggestions for incorporating all the varied forms of oral language within their lessons;' the speech-language therapist could mediate the communicative interaction between the teachers and pupils; and finally the speech-language therapist could assist in the implementation of the mma methodology initially as an expert consultant, and thereafter as a partner in a collaborative process. in this way the teacher and therapist together become facilitators of language, communication and learning. in summary, a facilitator of learning allows learners to work, learn and grow at their own pace, not according to the teachers' and therapists' preset time-table. the facilitator will allow for new and different ways to solve problems without the traditional limits. the facilitator will supply the resources that will enable the learner to find the meaning enabling them to focus on the process and not the product. the so-called "language-learning disabled population" can become innovative thinkers if their learning is facilitated within a geodesic environment using geodesic methodology recognising the possible neurological constraints of the language-learning disabled population, it is believed that within environments using geodesic frameworks such as the mma many of the problems of the language-learning disabled pupil can be overcome enabling them to become innovative lifelong learners. research (jensen, 1995; buzan, 1991; dhority, 1991; gardner, 1985) suggests that the brain thrives on novelty, challenge and enrichment, and therefore it is only logical and fair to put all types of learners into an environment that takes advantage of the natural functioning of the brain. according to feuerstein (1980), restricting the level of requirements of the language-learning disabled child by simplifying the environment and reducing challenges, will lower levels of motivation, aspiration and achievement. thus, in order to empower children with language-learning disabilities to reach higher levels of development, the traditional passive-acceptant approach must be replaced by an active approach to learning. according to this approach, the individual is an open system capable of mental and emotional modifiability. therefore, low levels of achievement are reversible and it is possible to learn efficiently if the proper effort is invested in diverse and integrated ways (feuerstein, 1980; jensen, 1995; gardner, 1985). in order to operationalise this implication, learning environments need to change, from being passive-acceptant to active-modification. entire new global systems need to be created that will allow all learners, whether language-learning disabled or not, to develop their potential together. a geodesic system of learning will focus on individuals and developing them; and not on fitting the individual into a system. separate schools for learning disabled students are not necessary, they are in fact making the situation worse. individual help can be given when required, but within the system. therefore the child with language-learning disabilities should be mainstreamed and not protected within isolated educational frameworks (simon, 1987). according to feuerstein (1989: 166), "the ultimate purpose is to bring him satisfaction, not by isolating him and avoiding confrontation, but rather by providing tools for the daily struggle with a normal environment in which he may achieve satisfaction". the philosophy 6f the geodesic information processing model, and its practical application, the mma, which is an active modification approach, has faith in the exceptional child's ability to change and grow, and accordingly great effort is invested in offering him many choices, as well as providing "tools" for change for example: the mind-map and the mma strategies. in addition, a system of "pull-out" programmes within the mainstream (simon, 1987) could be created for students with special needs. instead of the traditional approach which primarily teaches content more slowly, these should focus on the processes and values of learning, for instance, how to spell, rather than lists of rote spelling words; how to learn; mind-mapping; communication skills; and finally social skills (jensen, 1995). learning in a world of continuing accelerating change is a process of ongoing enquiry within the field of speech-language pathology and audiology, the increased awareness of the inefficiency of traditional approaches has led to the emergence of alternative treatment approaches. the alternative service delivery models (paul-brown, 1992) have been the result of speech-language therapists being required to serve a wider range of persons who present with a greater variety of communication disorders (lewis, 1994). this has resulted in a paradigm shift in the professional self-concept of and role played by the speech-language therapist. this necessitates the ability to learn to understand, guide, influence and manage these transformations or paradigm shifts. learning activity should be deformalized and replaced by flexible diversified models, such as the mma, based on the geodesic information processing model, in order to move learning into the twenty-first century (unesco, 1972, in ltfa, 1996). it therefore becomes an imperative task for individuals, institutions and society as a whole to learn about the process of learning in the attempt to overcome the maintenance effects of conditioned traditional paradigms, and to foster a climate of change, as well as to deal with the education and therapy crises, it is believed that the basic training of therapists and teachers needs to change to adopt a geodesic philosophy which allows for more flexible and diversified models to be created and implemented. the training of facilitators and, pupils or learners within geodesic philosophies such as the mma has to aim at changing attitudes in order to create global changes in traditional learning philosophies. this will have long-lasting effects on the skill level of application in teachers and therapists and their pupils and clients, and by implication, on the educational and therapeutic environments in which learning is facilitated. furthermore the institution of training programmes to achieve the objective of creating global changes to geodesic systems for education and therapy needs to recognise the complex interrelationship between the diverse institutional learning environments in order to be successful. if the philosophy of systems theory (von bertalanaffy, 1968 in leaf, 1997), which visualises the complex interaction of systems and sub-systems, is adopted, then geodesic frameworks such as the mma could be applied withthe southafrican journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a model for geodesic learning: • the larger framework of lifelong learning. the systems theory could therefore provide the principles of creating infrastructures within which geodesic frameworks could be implemented for education and therapy on all levels of learning. for instance knowles (1990) visualises an infrastructure for a lifelong learning resource system based on systems theory that emphasises the need to teach the community as a whole how to learn. the role of geodesic frameworks such as the mma in such an infrastructure is that of providing the "how" of the implementation of facilitating geodesic learning environments. thus, the systems theory (von bertalanaffy, 1968 in leaf, 1997) provides the infrastructure for the creation of geodesic learning environments, and programmes such as the mma provide the methods of training and facilitation within the geodesic learning environment. it is proposed that future research should concern itself with the interaction of the creation of geodesic infrastructures and the programmes providing the methods of facilitating geodesic learning. therefore the results of this study, and those of other similar research, need to be integrated with systems theory to create long-term and long-lasting changes that will ultimately equip learners with innovative lifelong learning skills. further research regarding the manner in which the attitudes of teachers, therapists, parents, pupils, and all those conditioned in to the traditional system can be enlightened in order to change their perceptions of their roles as learners. the purpose of "educating" and "remediating" is to facilitate innovative life skill learning competencies students should be excited about learning as it is a natural neuropsychological law that the brain is designed to learn. in a geodesic environment, students learn about life, they learn from each other, they learn what is in the curriculum and in therapy objectives, and they are ready to become lifelong learners that can contribute to society by applying geodesic philosophies, lifelong learning competencies can be developed. the geodesic information processing model and the practical application, the mma, focuses on the learner, not the content. the organisation of lesson and therapy objectives within the mma is based around creating conditions optimal for learning. it allows immersion into an integrated, thematic and interdisciplinary curriculum. this is in contrast to traditional formats of education and institution type therapy that emphasize learning one thing at a time so that a subject is divided into small chunks, and then sub-divided again and again. each day a micro chunk of the whole is presented out of context, for instance, "introduce unit a, learn it, take a test on it; now go to unit b" (jensen, 1995: 301). rather should one learn in an integrated thematic way. according to jensen (1995: 303), "our brain is designed to learn multi-path, in order, out of order, on many levels, with many teachers, in many contexts and from many angles. we learn with themes, favourite subjects, issues, key concepts, questions, trial and error and application. the thematic approach urges you to follow threads that weave through your student's world instead of a single subject or text book". this is the philosophy of the geodesic information processing model. the actual structure of the mind-map, the "tool" of the mma, promotes this geodesic information processing model 69 type of thinking because it creates patterns of meaning. therefore practical applications of geodesic philosophy such as the mma provide a strategic approach that can assist in the facilitation of innovative learners with good life-skills. conclusion the central thesis of the current paper is to change the perception of the traditional view of learning as a "mosaic of educational and therapeutic programmes conducted by a plethora of largely unconnected institutions" (knowles, 1990:17), into a lifelong learning resource system or learning community. this implies that learning should be viewed as an internal construction process controlled by the learner, as opposed to the internalisation of external facts from an external source such as a therapist or teacher. the geodesic model is seen to provide an alternative approach or system as well as a theoretical base to the perception of learning. the geodesic information processing model is also an attempt to develop a theory to explain why a geodesic framework such as the mma (not explained in this paper) invokes more effective thought processing than traditional behaviouristic approaches to education and therapy. the key issue in the geodesic information processing model of the current research is the intimate interaction and interdependence of metacognition and neuropsychology. the model proposes that metacognition is the nonconscious level, elevating metacognition to the level where most learning (approximately 90%) occurs. this is the highest level of thought, where thinking begins. the model then proposes that cognition is the next level of thought, the level of conscious thinking responsible for approximately 10% of learning. both levels need to be fully activated according to the ratio of their responsibility in order for learning to be effective and result in usefully reconceptualised knowledge. if methodologies and systems are used that are incompatible with natural neuropsychological laws, then the cognitive level will be predominantly activated, with limited intermittent involvement of the metacognitive non-conscious level. this will result in inefficient rote-type learning with a product orientation as opposed to process orientation. it is believed that geodesic methodologies such as the mma (leaf, 1997), are neuropsychologically based and will thus activate metacognition and cognition in the correct way . in contrast, traditional methodologies stimulate predominantly cognitive processes with concomitant learning limitations. furthermore, the mind-map itself is viewed as the "tool" which directly accesses and trains the metacognitive non-conscious. the emphasis of the mma is on the facilitation of improved language, learning and communication through a strategic versus skill-based approach communication, language and learning are seen as being controlled by metacognition , which will in turn influence information processing and thus the effectiveness of communication, language and learning oral or written. in other words, strategies are being facilitated at the root level and once automatized, will have a more effective result in terms of generalisation than if fragmented skills are trained. "throughout history only a few people have benefited from the growing corpus of scientific knowledge which permits the development of human potential. inequality die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 caroline μ. leaf, isabel uys & brenda louw of human beings was not determined genetically. they all have more or less the same potentiality , the same capacity to think" (van der vyver & capdeveille, 1990:6). therefore, every effort should be made to develop this potential of individuals. everyone should be allowed the opportunity to learn how to learn. acknowledgements the current article is based on the d.phil, research of c. leaf. the financial assistance of the centre for science development towards the research is hereby gratefully acknowledged. references allport, d.a. (1980). patterns and actions: cognitive mechanisms and content specific in claxton, g.l. (ed), cognitive psychology: new directions. london: routledge & kegan paul. dhority, l. (1991). the act approach: the artful use of suggestion for integrative learning. bremen, west germany: pls verlag gmbh, an der weide. diamond, m. (1988). enriching heredity: the impact of the environment on the brain. new york: free press. feldman, d. (1980). beyond uniuersals in cognitive development. norwood, n.j.: ablex publishers. feuerstein, r. (1980). instrumental enrichment: an intervention programme for cognitive modifiability. baltimore, maryland: university park press. flavell, j.h. (1978). metacognitive development in scandura j.m. & brainerd, c.j. (eds) structural / process / theories of complete human behaviour. the netherlands: sijthoff & noordoff. gardner, h. (1985). frames of mind. new york: basic books. glasser, m.d. (1986). control theory in the classroom. new york: harper & row. goldberg, e. & costa, l.d. (1981). hemisphere differences in the acquisition and use of descriptive systems. brain and language, 14 144-173. hinton, g.e. & anderson, j.a. (1981). parallel models of associate memory. hillsdale n.j.: erlsbaum. holt, j. (1964). how children fail. new york: pitman. iran-nejad, a. (1990). active and dynamic self-regulation of learning processes. review of educational research, 60 (4) 573-602. jensen, e. (1995). brain-based learning and teaching. south africa : process graphix johnson, j.m. (1987). a case history of professional evolution from slp to communication instructor. journal of childhood communication disorders, 11 (4) 225-234. king, d.f. & goodman, k.s. (1990). whole language learning, cherishing learners and their language. language, speech and hearing sciences in schools, 21 221-229. kline, p. (1990) everyday genius. arlington, v.a.: great ocean publishers. knowles, m. (1990) the adult learner: a neglected species. houston: gulf publishing company. leaf, c.m. (1990). mind-mapping: a therapeutic technique for closed head injury. unpublished masters dissertation, university of pretoria. leaf, c.m. (1997). the mind mapping approach: a model and framework for geodesic learning. unpublished d.phil dissertation, university of pretoria. leaf, c.m., uys, i.c. & louw, b. (1993). mind mapping: a culture and language free technique. south african journal of communication disorders, 40 35-43. lewis, r. (1994). report back on the workshop: speech / language / hearing therapy in transition. communiphon, 308 6-7. lozanov, g. (1978). suggestology and outlines of suggestopedy. new york: gordon and breach science publishers. luria, a.r. (1980). higher cortical functions in man (2nd ed). new york: basic books. paul-brown, d. (1992). professional practices perspective on alternative service delivery models. asha bulletin, 12. schory, m.e. (1990). whole language and the speech. language pathologists. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 21 206-211. simon, c.s. (1987). out of the boom closet and into the classroom: the emerging slp. journal of childhood communication disorders, 11 (1-2) 81-90. thurman, s.k. & widerstrom, a.h. (1990). infants and young children with special needs a developmental and ecological approach. 2nd (ed) baltimore: paul h. brookes publishing co. van der vyver, d.w. & de capdeveille, b. (1990). towards the mountain: characteristics and implications of the south african upptrail pilot project. journal of the society for accelerative learning and teaching, 15 (1 & 2) 59-74. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) employment in the uk for a working holiday or permanent position, b b t is second to n o n e . join the hundreds of speech and language therapists we secure locum and long term positions for each year. our excellent reputation allows us to offer assignments throughout the uk, many of which are in the leading establishments in the state and private sectors. our dedicated team can guide you through the registration procedures prior to arrival and once in the uk, provide you with all the support you require. you will find our experience invaluable in ensuring your time is a complete success and the overall package unbeatable. contact sean ridgwell in london today for an initial discussion and our free information pack, or send your details, so that we can contact you. γ b e r e s f o r d b l a k e τ η 0 μ a s i. τ d toll free-0800 998 154 fax: 09 44 171 233 8004 e-mail: recruit@bbt.co.uk 14 buckingham palace road, london sw1w 0qp, england. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mailto:recruit@bbt.co.uk r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) undergraduate clinical training in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction in sa 5 clinical training of undergraduate communication pathology students in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction in south africa alta kritzinger and brenda louw clinic for high risk babies (chrib), centre for early intervention in communication pathology (ceicp), department of communication pathology, university of pretoria abstract developing an undergraduate clinical training module in early communication intervention (eci) that provides sufficient opportunity for students' learning experiences, and that allows contextually relevant and ethically accountable services to clients, as well as the dissemination of the latest scientific findings to both students and clients, poses a significant challenge. the article describes the development of a clinical training block and the development of an appropriate instrument for the assessment of neonatal communication skills that was used in this student practical. a qualitative research approach, employing a series of formative assessments, was used to refine the instrument. the results indicated changes in the content and use of the neonatal assessment instrument regarding the approach to infant and caregiver risk assessment; caregiver beliefs about neonatal capabilities; newborn state observation; caregiver training; language, cultural and age barriers; collaboration with nurses; kangaroo mother care; involvement of caregivers during the assessment; and adolescent mothers. the results confirmed the need for the ongoing refinement of an instrument to ensure quality training of students in the difficult field of neonatal assessments and neonate-caregiver interaction. the results also highlighted to the importance of initiating eci services before birth in communities at risk for infant neglect and abuse, and the close relationship between eci service delivery and student training. introduction early communication intervention (eci) has evolved as an important and new field within speechlanguage pathology and audiology internationally. the field was introduced to speech-language pathology curricula in south africa following the south african speech-language hearing association (saslha) position statement on eci (louw, 1997) and decisions taken by the different departments at the u n i v e r s i t i e s which offer s p e e c h l a n g u a g e p a t h o l o g y and a u d i o l o g y p r o g r a m m e s . t h e university of pretoria started formal clinical training in eci in 1990 when the (clinic for high risk babies (chrib) was establishedj(kritzinger & louw, 2002). since the introduction of eci in south africa, its training has been influenced by changes in the profession that were prompted by the political and societal transition in the country. during the past decade, various authors jwithin the field of speechlanguage pathology andi audiology in south africa have advocated a transformation in tertiary teaching programmes in order to meet the demand of providing speech-language services to all those who require intervention (kritzinger, louw & hugo, 1995; moodley, louw & hugo, 2000; pillay, kathard & samuel, 1997). in this regard, the importance of tertiary education as the only basis for effective and high standard services (uys, 1993), and the ideal that teaching should anticipate and react to the needs of clients, was emphasized (uys & hugo, 1997). furthermore, the implementation of appropriate service delivery models based on community needs and resources (fair & louw, 1999; tuomi, 1994) and a s t r o n g e m p h a s i s on p r e v e n t i o n and e a r l y intervention was proposed (hugo, 1998; louw, 1997). eci training has also been influenced by changes in tertiary level teaching approaches, characterized by learner-centredness, relevance, integration, critical and creative thinking, and a holistic approach to learner assessment (gultig, lubisi, parker & wedekind, 1998). the new education approach moved away from formal lectures to outcomes-based education, and represents a radical departure from a rigid separation between a contentbased education component and a training arena that involves the gaining of skills (le grange & reddy, 1998). the implications for eci curriculum design are that the traditional dichotomy of theoretical knowledge and its application in practice should be viewed as an integrated process, attempting to create authentic learning experiences both in the classroom and in clinical settings. consequently, the outcomes of authentic learning experiences in eci involve c o n s i d e r a b l y more than the a c c u m u l a t i o n of knowledge, and include the achievement of relevant skills, as well as shaping learners' attitudes and values regarding families requiring eci, their communities, the collaborators in the process and the advancement of the field of eci in south africa. since learning, teaching and assessment are inextricably linked in an outcomes-based education curriculum, the centrality of learners and their knowledge, experiences and needs should also be reflected in the assessment procedures that are selected (gultig et al., 1998). although several components of a coherent assessment system are described in outcomes-based education, the element of formative assessment appears to be particularly a d v a n t a g e o u s for c u r r i c u l u m d e s i g n in eci. formative assessment can be conducted while the learning process takes place and may be used to influence and inform both the learning process as well as the teaching process. the learner is provided with 1 based on a poster presentation at the 4 l h graz symposium on developmental neurology, may 22 to 24, 2003, austria. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) alta kritzinger and brenda louw 6 guidance in the form of self-assessment and feedback through peer-assessment and teacher-assessment. since learners themselves are regarded as sources of knowledge, this kind of assessment also guides the teacher's planning and allows for the critique of outcomes, methodology and materials (gultig et al., 1998; le grange & reddy, 1998). f o r m a t i v e assessments can therefore be used to develop an eci curriculum responsive to the needs of the learners as well as the needs of the clients requiring eci services, which can increase the standard and effectiveness of eci training and the services that are provided. since many infants and young children in south africa are at risk for developmental delay, neglect and abuse due to low birth weight and poverty related factors (central statistical services, 1997), the newborn period offers a unique opportunity to i n t e r v e n e e a r l y in o r d e r to p r e v e n t a d v e r s e developmental outcomes in infants. whilst the mothers and their newborn infants are still in hospital after delivery and in the beginning stages of postnatal attachment, the first few days of an infant's life are important to provide information to caregivers and to p o s s i b l y p r e v e n t n e g l e c t , a b u s e and d e l a y e d development. by demonstrating the remarkable communication interaction capabilities and listening skills of newborns to the caregivers, they may also be alerted to the importance of responsive interaction to facilitate the infants' early development. at the same time, students can be trained in the complex field of n e o n a t a l a s s e s s m e n t s and n e o n a t e c a r e g i v e r interaction by applying a neonatal assessment instrument upon which inferences of a newborn infant's communication behaviour can be made and demonstrated to the mother in order to enhance the infant's communication development. developing an undergraduate clinical training module in eci that provides sufficient opportunity for students' learning experiences in contexts of increased complexity, that allows the provision of contextually relevant and ethically accountable services to clients, as well as the dissemination of the latest scientific findings to both students and clients, poses a significant challenge to curriculum developers. the main aim of the article is to describe how a specific clinical training block was developed as part of a module in the eci curriculum. central to the development of the clinical block was the design of an appropriate clinical tool for the assessment of neonatal communication skills by undergraduate students. the sub-aim is therefore to describe the p r o c e s s of c l i n i c a l l y t r a i n i n g u n d e r g r a d u a t e communication pathology students in a public hospital outside pretoria in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction by using the neonatal communication assessment instrument (kritzinger, 1994; see appendix a). method students' written reports on the clinical application of the assessment instrument during their annual practical training were analysed by means of a series of formative evaluations (fouche, 2002; gultig et al., 1998; mitchell, 1991). as the aim of the research was to evaluate processes, practices and outcomes in order to d e c i d e on m o d i f i c a t i o n s of the neonatal communication assessment instrument, a formative evaluation process was used. the formative evaluation process involved comparing the students' reports with the stated outcomes of the training module in order to identify limitations in the assessment instrument (le grange & reddy, 1998). since the initial design of the instrument in 1994, the neonatal assessments carried out annually by each cohort of students in the same p u b l i c h o s p i t a l was followed by a formative evaluation process and the application of changes to the i n s t r u m e n t , thereby creating a c o n t i n u o u s feedback loop in order to refine the neonatal communication assessment instrument and its clinical application. participants neonatal assessments have formed part of an e c i p r a c t i c a l b l o c k at t h e d e p a r t m e n t of communication pathology, university of pretoria since 1995 (see a p p e n d i x b). the formative evaluation process was conducted during the period of 1995 to 2002 and between 6 and 40 second year students were annually involved as participants in the study (see table 1). the students were informed that their written reports on their learning experiences with neonatal assessments would be used in a research project in order to refine the assessment instrument and to improve the authenticity of the learning experience. students who gave individual informed consent to participate in the research were requested to submit their reports once marks had been allocated following completion of the module. the average age of second year students was 20 years. while all the second year s t u d e n t s were r e q u i r e d to p a r t i c i p a t e in the compulsory training module, which required themjto learn and gain experience in the use of the neonatal communication assessment instrument, but only some students agreed to participate in the study (see table 1). . 1 ' ι i tablel. numbers of students completing the i training module and participating in the { study. ι year students students completing the module acting as participants 1995 36 7 1996 30 6 1997 36 8 1998 34 9 / 1999 39 10 2000 43 χ ' 9 2001 47 42' 2002 44 , 39 total 309 121 a phenomenological approach was employed as a qualitative research strategy to develop and refine the neonatal communication assessment instrument. in addition, a naturalistic method of study was used to describe the meaning of experiences recorded by students during their practical training (fouche, 2002). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieeafwykings, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) undergraduate clinical training in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction in sa 7 materials a comprehensive neonatal communication assessment instrument which includes the following components, was compiled (see appendix a): β prenatal, perinatal and environmental risk factors (kritzinger et al., 1995). a checklist was used to determine the mother-infant dyad risk profile. this p a r t i c u l a r risk assessment is intended for neonates born at term, and although the perinatal risks associated with infants born preterm and with low birth weight are not expected, it is still important to identify prenatal and perinatal conditions that may possibly increase an infant's risk for communication delay (rossetti, 2001). θ mother-infant communication interaction (klein & briggs, 1987). since the earliest interactions between a mother and neonate set the stage for communication development (billeaud, 1998), the quality of neonate-caregiver interaction should be assessed in order to intervene as early as possible. © neonatal states (brazelton, 1984). a neonate's state of alertness depends on physiological and environmental factors that should be utilized as a lens through which samples of b e h a v i o u r obtained during the assessment are interpreted (rossetti, 2001). ο feeding behaviour (wolf & glass, 1992). sine early feeding difficulties have been found to be predictive of later communication development (kritzinger, 1994) and are considered to be one of the risk factors for communication disorders (rossetti, 2001) an assessment of the manner and quality of the feeding process is imperative, ο precursors to language content, form and use. early interactive abilities and listening skills of newborns suggest that they are "prewired" for communication, and are related to the neonates' f u t u r e c o m m u n i c a t i o n d e v e l o p m e n t in content/form/use interactions (lahey, 1988; / o w e n s , 2001). thesejneonatal abilities should be / assessed as they enable infants to participate actively in reciprocal, interaction with their mothers. the absence of these typical behaviours in term newborns should be investigated further in order to identify risks for communication disorders and hearing impairment as early as possible. j θ precursors to cognition. since cognitive and communication development'are closely related, it is important to assess the early adaptive behaviours displayed by neonates (owens, 2001) in order to obtain a comprehensive sample of neonatal communication skills. the assessment instrument was designed for neonates from birth to 28 days old, and their mothers, and the assessment was based on the presence or absence of neonatal and caregiver behaviours, observed or elicited by the examiner. the instrument was used to assess the communication skills of neonates born at term, to assess the mother-infant interaction and to provide the mother with feedback on her infant's communication c a p a b i l i t i e s and r e a d i n e s s for c o m m u n i c a t i o n interaction. the mothers and their newborn infants resided in the 40-bed maternity ward of kalafong hospital, a large tertiary care hospital situated on the outskirts of pretoria. the hospital is utilized by the university of pretoria as a training site for students from different disciplines. the students' training block in the maternity ward of this hospital formed part of a prevention programme for speech-language and hearing problems in neonates. validity and reliability in order to enhance the face validity of the neonatal communication assessment instrument the tool was structured in such a way as to measure the different a s p e c t s of n e o n a t a l c o m m u n i c a t i o n behaviours accurately (kritzinger, 1994). based on a literature review and the careful consideration of different theoretical models, content validity was adhered to by including an adequate sample of items that represent the concept of neonatal communication abilities and ensuring that all the items actually describe this central concept. the assessment instrument appeared to be reliable since independent administrations over the years by students yielded c o n s i s t e n t r e s u l t s of n e o n a t a l c o m m u n i c a t i o n behaviours (de vos & fouche, 1998). data collection and analysis the written reports of each cohort of students were collected every year and the data was analyzed and interpreted using the huberman and mile's approach to data analysis in qualitative research (poggenpoel, 1998). the reports contained written descriptions of observations and reflections on the learning experiences. using the different components of the neonatal communication assessment instrument and the outcomes of the training module as guidelines, the large volumes of data were condensed, clustered and displayed according to themes. as a result of this process the assessment instrument was continually adapted and the latest version is presented in appendix a. since the first author was also the on-site clinical instructor during the training module, her own observations and theoretical guidelines served as multiple methods of data collection in order to increase the reliability of observations (de vos, 1998). although the first author became part of the process, care was taken to avoid influencing the participants' opinions written in their reports. after the practical brief on-site discussions were held with the students in order to facilitate reflection on the theoretical and clinical implications of their learning experiences. the d i s c u s s i o n s were c o n d u c t e d by e l i c i t i n g comments from the students and responding to their remarks and not by informing them what they were supposed to have gained from the practical. in order to interpret the participants' reports accurately, careful analysis of the data was necessary. this p r o c e s s i n v o l v e d c l o s e s c r u t i n y of the participants' use of terminology and language that often revealed their inexperience with the subject matter. the displayed data was interpreted according to themes by means of inductive abstraction and generalization (poggenpoel, 1998). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) alta kritzinger and brenda louw 8 results and discussion the results indicated the need for changes in the content and use of the neonatal communication assessment instrument regarding the following themes: • the approach to infant and caregiver risk assessment risk assessment has traditionally been the s t a r t i n g p o i n t for the early i d e n t i f i c a t i o n of communication disorders and hearing impairment (rossetti, 2001; kritzinger, 2000). initially, only biological risk factors in the mother and infant were considered in compiling a risk profile of the caregiver-infant dyad. the most prevalent biological risk factors identified over the years were adolescent mothers, multiparous mothers with four or more children, maternal hiv/aids and low birth weight in the infants. all of these risk factors may be associated with poverty conditions that can negatively impact on the mother's ability to care for the infant or to stimulate the infant's development sufficiently (rossetti, 2001). the identification of these four biological risk factors in the mother-neonate dyads, led to the inclusion of environmental risk factors, such as maternal educational level, employment, and quality of housing (samerhoff, seifer, baldwin & baldwin, 1993) in the assessment instrument (see appendix a). it became clear that an integrated approach to the risk assessment of the caregiver-infant dyad should be followed, since the different risk factors are interrelated (garbarino & ganzel, 2000). furthermore, it became clear that positive n e o n a t e c a r e g i v e r interaction and the mothers' enthusiastic responses to the information they received should be viewed as protective factors for further development of the infant (osofsky & thompson, 2000). although long-term predictions based on early assessment results of infants are not possible (rossetti, 2001), it is important to identify risk and protective factors in order to support and encourage parents to continue to enhance their infants' d e v e l o p m e n t , e s p e c i a l l y when an a s s e t b a s e d approach to intervention is followed (ammerman & parks, 1998). as a result of the increased awareness of the m u l t i p l e r i s k factors and the c o m p l e x i t y of interactions between risk factors and protecting factors affecting the communication development of newborns in kalafong hospital, the need for a longterm prevention programme became clear. in order to design such a contextually relevant programme, caregiver views on neonatal behaviours needed to be considered. • caregiver beliefs about neonatal capabilities u n d e r s t a n d i n g c a r e g i v e r p e r c e p t i o n s regarding neonatal capabilities is crucial if they are to be successfully incorporated in eci. the eci client base at kalafong hospital consists mostly of women living in a nearby township and surrounding areas, and who utilize the state-subsidized health care services provided at the hospital. many of the caregivers, even though they were m u l t i p a r o u s mothers, believed that their newborn infants were not able to see and hear at birth and that they acquire these abilities only after a few months. the implications of these perceptions could be that mothers are not responsive to their infants or that they may not attribute meaning to their young infants' early communicative signals. louw and avenant (2002) identified beliefs and perceptions regarding infants, communication interaction styles in families and child rearing patterns in different cultures in south africa as factors influencing caregivers' responses to early intervention services. since these beliefs represent the reality for the mothers and their communities, it is important to deal with the beliefs in a sensitive manner and not to judge the mothers or their perceptions. the finding that mothers were unaware of their newborns' c a p a b i l i t i e s led to the active involvement of the mother in the assessment process (brazelton, 1984) and demonstration to her that the infant had a preference for her voice instead of the assessor's voice, responded to all kinds of sounds, made eye contact and imitated facial movements. depending on the students' skills to elicit and demonstrate responsive interaction with the neonates (salmon, rowan & mitchell, 1998), the mothers usually responded with surprise, and their increased interest in their infants could still be observed at the end of an afternoon's session. although the long-term benefits for the neonates' communication development was beyond the scope of the current research project, the prevention programme for speech-language and hearing problems in neonates in the maternity ward of kalafong hospital, suggested the need for a multilevel framework for eci public service delivery in south africa. consequently a conceptual framework was developed, incorporating c o m m u n i t y b a s e d and institution-based models of service delivery, with a strong focus on p r e v e n t i o n of c o m m u n i c a t i o n disorders in young children. the framework, illustrated in table 2, includes the basic components of an early intervention service delivery system (fair & louw, 1999) but also indicates the different contexts where caregivers and their young infants may be identified, what the various professional functions in these contexts may entail and who are the different collaborative partners for information exchange. lastly, the framework makes provision for the long-term follow-up of young children and their caregivers at different levels of care, beginning in the antenatal period, where expectant mothers may be alerted to the importance of their unborn children's future communication and literacy development, and advancing through the neonatal and postnatal periods to the toddler years. as indicated in table 2 the shaded portion highlights the neonatal period when the practical block described in this article is undertaken every year in the maternity ward of kalafong hospital. / • language, cultural and age barriers south africa's multicultural and multilingual populations lead to barriers in service delivery, and present a challenge to eci clinicians to communicate effectively with their clients,(louw & avenant, 2002). the students and mothers often did not share the same die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieeafwykings, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) undergraduate clinical training in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction in sa 9 table 2. conceptual framework for early communication intervention public service delivery in south africa service delivery model according to child's age context where caregivers can be reached functions of professional involvement collaborative partners forinformation exchange before infant is born: communitybased antenatal care clinic at primary health care centre raising awareness of: normal communication and literacy development importance of stimulation pregnant mothers community nurses speech-language therapists / early interventionists neonatal period: institution-based maternity ward in hospital neonatal intensive care unit determine neonate's risk profile screen neonate's communication and hearing abilities facilitate interest in neonatal communication abilities and developmental needs train mother to stimulate early development mothers and fathers extended family members nurses in maternity ward speech-language therapists / early interventionists audiologists postnatal period: communitybased immunization clinic at primary health care centre serial screening of communication skills identify risks and provide intervention reinforce interest in stimulating infant's communication and early literacy development advocacy for education parents / secondary caregivers community nurses speech-language therapists / early interventionists audiologists toddler years: communitybased day care centre nursery school provide intervention for children with communication delays train parents for ongoing stimulation of child's communication development facilitate the implementation of a language and literacy-based preschool curriculum to ensure school readiness parents secondary caregivers nursery school teachers speech-language therapists / early interventionists audiologists l a n g u a g e , or p r e s e n t e d with limited language proficiency of a shared language and no interpreters were available in the maternity ward. the caregivers often presented with low functional literacy as well. the age difference between some of the caregivers and.students was often a communication barrier as people from certain cultural traditions are accustomed to impart knowledge from the old to the young, and not vice versa. these aspects remain challenges, but as the student-caregiver, interaction relied on the demonstration of the newborn's capabilities, which invariably elicited positive reactions from the caregivers, an atmosphere conducive to improved communication could bei created. the advantage for f u r t h e r eci t r a i n i n g in a m u l t i c u l t u r a l and multilingual context was that students had already been exposed to some problem-based learning experiences in clinical situations early in their curriculum. © kangaroo mother care kangaroo mother care is currently viewed as best practice for all neonates in all contexts (power, 2002). although kalafong hospital runs a very successful kangaroo mother care programme for premature infants (van rooyen, pullen, pattinson, & delport, 2002), the practice did not include the fullterm neonates in the maternity ward. as a result of the students' previous exposure to kangaroo mother care in another module, and i having seen its application with premature infants in the kangaroo mother care unit of the same hospital, it was clear that the benefits of this neonatal care technique could not be ignored in communities where abandonment and neglect of infants occurs. this technique has proven to benefit the health of the neonate, lactation and mother-infant attachment, which forms the basis of communication development (hann, malan, kronson, bergman & huskisson, 1999; kritzinger & louw, 1999). in collaboration with the nursing staff, kangaroo mother care was introduced to the mothers of term neonates, especially those with low birth weight and/or small for gestational age infants, and to adolescent mothers in the maternity ward. the training advantage was that students were able to identify a need and apply their knowledge of recent scientific findings in order to find a practical solution in a real world clinical context. • newborn state observation identifying the six different newborn states (brazelton, 1984) is a basic skill to be achieved when conducting neonatal assessments. since healthy newborns are mostly in an alert state during the first 60-90 minutes after birth, and then mostly in a sleeping state (rossetti, 2001), it proved to be difficult to find the infant in a quiet alert state to conduct an interactive communication assessment. since the students were not experienced in eliciting and maintaining alert states in the infants, it was often not possible to administer the complete the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) alta kritzinger and brenda louw 10 assessment instrument. in order to improve the students' skills in identifying the neonatal states, additional time was allocated to prepare for the practical block and to watch video material on neonatal behaviour in clinical seminars. β collaboration with nurses since an orientation to teamwork represents one of the principles of practice in eci, the aim was to include the contributions of multiple disciplines (guralnick, 1997). to integrate and sustain the eci services initiated by speech-language therapists in the activities of the maternity ward, the value of building trusting relationships between nurses and therapists was shown to be beneficial to the prevention programme. the students commented on the nurses' willingness to offer assistance and to introduce them to the mothers. the implications of these positive interactions with the nurses were that students were introduced to collaborative work with members of the health team at an early stage in their training (briggs, 1997; moodley et al., 2000). β adolescent mothers s i n c e t h e p r e v a l e n c e of a d o l e s c e n t pregnancies in south africa is high (department of health, 1997) this group of caregivers deserves special attention in eci. the students became acutely aware of the parenting risks for adolescent mothers (osofsky & thompson, 2000; rossetti, 2001) as they shared the same stage of life with these mothers. it was clear that there were personal gains as well as academic advantages for the students in training adolescent mothers who were often disinterested in their infants and lacked basic knowledge about infant d e v e l o p m e n t . the s t u d e n t s r e p o r t e d that the a d o l e s c e n t m o t h e r s often c o m m u n i c a t e d more spontaneously with them than some of the older mothers. © training of caregivers during the assessment caregiver training is the cornerstone of eci (rossetti, 2001) and should be a priority skill to be achieved in student training. since the students were still at a junior level of training (second year) and a fair amount of academic preparation was involved in the application of the assessment instrument, a caregiver-training package was developed which they could use with their beginner's skills. the training package consisted of a poster translated into three languages, namely english, setswana and afrikaans, and a brochure which could be used to reinforce information about newborn auditory, visual, feeding and communication interactive capabilities, and developmental needs. the training package may be used to facilitate the process of eci or serve as a. model that can be adapted to suit different contexts. the application of the caregiver-training package acted as one of the underpinnings for the students' further clinical training in providing familycentered eci services and learning experiences where increased learner responsibility was required. practical block in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction as part of a module in the eci curricul um t h e r e s u l t s of t h e s t u d y led to t h e development of a learner-centered practical block in n e o n a t a l a s s e s s m e n t a n d n e o n a t e c a r e g i v e r interaction in which the neonatal communication assessment instrument was an integral component. the practical block, supported by three preparatory clinical seminars, was a small component of the entire eci curriculum, where the outcomes allowed for a gradual increase in the students' responsibility to manage their own learning experiences, from the protected environment of the classroom to the various clinical settings. simultaneously, the outcomes of the eci curriculum facilitated a systematic decrease in supervisor support and clinical instruction over the four years of study (see appendix c). the shaded area i n d i c a t e s the scope, e s s e n t i a l k n o w l e d g e and outcomes of the practical block in the second year of study, which was described in the article. the content and clinical training component of the practical block in n e o n a t a l a s s e s s m e n t and n e o n a t e c a r e g i v e r interaction was dynamic and was adjusted annually in response to student assessment of the learning experiences, new developments in the field and new texts that were published. the outcomes of the practical block were based on a set of characteristics relating to the practical training in the eci c u r r i c u l u m at the d e p a r t m e n t of communication pathology, university of pretoria, and these features are presented as follows: 1. outcomes are demonstrations of competence and students are expected to use essential knowledge and carefully planned learning experiences in clearly defined performance contexts to achieve the desired competencies for each learning unit/module. 2. learning tasks and outcomes become increasingly more complex as students progress over the four years of study. 3. the outcomes of the junior years of study act as enabling outcomes or stepping-stones to the ultimate outcomes in the final year of study. 4. students accumulate learning experiences during every year of study and use all the prior knowledge, skills and developing professional conduct in each new learning task, resembling a spiral of learning. , 5. the scope of praxis increases over time to include different service delivery contexts, and provides various opportunities for rich learning experiences. 7. t h e r e is a g r a d u a l i n c r e a s e in l e a r n e r responsibility for managing own learning experiences with a gradual decrease in supervisor support and clinical instruction over the four years of study. ^ 8. the ultimate goal is for students to use their knowledge, skills and professional conduct to be innovative, competent, adaptable and self-motivated in eci service delivery in south africa. (based on killen, 1999; spady, killen & rand, 2000) figure i was compiled to demonstrate that authentic learning experiences in eci clinical training depend on a carefully designed curriculum as well as a contextually appropriate service/delivery model, as presented in appendix c and table 2. since the aim of clinical training was carefiilly designed learning experiences in a real world context, an interdependent relationship existed between the curriculum and service delivery that anticipated and responded to eci client needs. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieeafwykings, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) undergraduate clinical training in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction in sa 11 carefully d e s i g n e d p r a c t i c a l b l o c k w i t h i n the eci c u r r i c u l u m figure i: interdependent components of authentic learning experiences in eci clinical training conclusion the results confirmed the need for the ongoing refinement of an instrument to enhance the quality training of students in the difficult field of n e o n a t a l a s s e s s m e n t s and n e o n a t e c a r e g i v e r interaction. it appeared that it was not only essential to design a valid and reliable assessment instrument, but also to develop appropriate strategies and techniques to administer the assessment tool in specific contexts. there is a dearth of clinical training instruments with relevant procedures in eci that can be u s e d in the s o u t h a f r i c a n c o n t e x t . t h e development of the content and procedures of the neonatal communication assessment instrument was an attempt to bridge the gap between theory and practice in learning, and to present a clinically proven tool in an accessible format. the development of the training block represented one of the enabling steps in the spiral of learning created by the entire eci curriculum over four years of study. the results also emphasized the importance of the relationship between clinical teaching in eci and the needs of the communities that provide these rich learning experiences. in order to maintain a prevention focus, it is most important to initiate eci services not only from birth onwards, but also from before birth in those communities at risk for infant neglect and abuse. therefore, authentic learning experiences for studentsj should be comprehensive, contextually relevant, ethically accountable and d i s s e m i n a t e t h e l a t e s t s c i e n t i f i c f i n d i n g s appropriately. such learning experiences must be supported by a carefully designed curriculum and guided by a conceptual framework of service delivery. this report illustrated how state of the art and advanced theory on neonatal development could be translated to practical applications in a clinical block in order to make a difference at the level of mothers and their newborn infants in kalafong hospital. references ammerman, α., & parks, c. (1998). preparing s t u d e n t s for m o r e e f f e c t i v e c o m m u n i t y interventions: assets assessment. family and community health, 21(2), 32-45. billeaud, f. p. (1998). communication disorders in infants and toddlers.^assessment and intervention (2n d ed.). boston: butterworth-heinemann. brazelton, τ. b. (1984). neonatal behavioural assessment scale (2 n d ed.). london: spastics international medical publications. briggs, μ. h. (1997). building early intervention teams. gaithersburg: aspen publishers, inc. central statistical services. (1997). october household survey 1995. p r e t o r i a : c e n t r a l statistical services. d e p a r t m e n t of health. ( 1 9 9 7 ) . white paper: transformation of the health system. [retrieved november 1, 1997, from http://www.anc.org.za.] de vos, a. s. (1998). combined quantitative and qualitative approach. in a. s. de vos (ed.), research at grass roots. a primer for the caring professions, (pp. 357-363). pretoria: j. l. van schaik academic. de vos, a. s., & fouche, c. b. (1998). general introduction to research design, data collection methods and data analysis. in a. s. de vos. (ed.), research at grass roots. a primer for the caring professions (pp. 76-94). pretoria: j. l. van schaik academic. fair, l., & louw, b. (1999). early communication i n t e r v e n t i o n w i t h i n a c o m m u n i t y b a s e d intervention model in south africa. south african journal of communication disorders, 46,13-23. fouche, c. b. (2002). research strategies. in a. s. de vos, h. strydom, c. b. fouche, & c. s. l. delport (eds.), research at grass roots. for the social sciences and human service professions. (2 n d ed., pp. 270-277). pretoria: van schaik publishers. garbarino, j., & ganzel, b. (2000). the human ecology of risk. in j. p. shonkoff & s. j. meisels (eds.). handbook of early childhood intervention (2 n d ed., pp. 76-93). cambridge: cambridge university press. gultig, j., lubisi, c., parker, b., & wedekind, v. (1 9 9 8 ) . understanding outcomes-based education. teaching and assessment in south africa. cape town: oxford university press. guralnick, μ j. (1997). the effectiveness of early intervention (2n d ed.), baltimore: paul η brookes publishing company. hann, m., malan, α., kronson, m., bergman, n., & huskisson, j. (1999). kangaroo mother care. south african medical journal, 89(1), 37-39. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) http://www.anc.org.za alta kritzinger and brenda louw 12 hugo, s. r. (1998). communication pathology: the way in africa. the south african journal of communication disorders, 45, 3-9. klein, m. d., & briggs, μ. h. (1987). observation of communicative interaction. journal of childhood communication disorders, 10, 95-106. killen, r. (1999, june-july). using the saqa critical outcomes to inform curriculum planning in higher education in south africa. paper presented at the b i e n n i a l conference of the south african association for research and development, belville, western cape. k r i t z i n g e r , a . m . ( 1 9 9 4 ) . v r o e e k o m m u n i k a s i e o n t w i k k e l i n g van b i o l o g i e s e r i s i k o b a b a s . u n p u b l i s h e d m a s t e r s t h e s i s , university of pretoria, gauteng, south africa. kritzinger, a. m. (2000). establishing a computerbased data system for early communication i n t e r v e n t i o n in south africa. u n p u b l i s h e d doctoral dissertation, university of pretoria, gauteng, south africa. kritzinger, α., louw, b., & hugo, r. (1995). communication skills of biologically, at-risk n e o n a t e s . the south african journal of communication disorders, 42, 7-1 7. kritzinger, α., & louw, b. (1999). kangaroo mother care: a strategy to facilitate mother-infant communication interaction in high-risk infants in different c o n t e x t s . [ m o n o g r a p h ] clinica: applications in clinical practice of communication pathology: 4, 31-46. kritzinger, α., & louw, b. (2002). a comprehensive assessment protocol for infants and toddlers at risk for c o m m u n i c a t i o n disorders. part ii: assessment protocol. [monograph] clinica: applications in clinical practice of communication pathology. 6, 3-17. lahey, m. (1988). language disorders and language development. new york: macmillan. le grange, l., & reddy, c. (1998). continuous assessment. kenwyn: juta & company limited. louw, b. (1997). early communication intervention: guidelines for speech-language therapists and audiologists. johannesburg: saslha ethics and standards committee. louw, b., & avenant, c. (2002). culture as context for i n t e r v e n t i o n : d e v e l o p i n g a c u l t u r a l l y c o n g r u e n t e a r l y i n t e r v e n t i o n p r o g r a m . international pediatrics, 17(3), 145-150. lubchenko, l. o. (1987). assessment of weight and gestational age. in g. b. avery (ed.), neonatology: pathophysiology and management of the newborn. philadelphia: j. b. lippincott. mitchell, d. r. (1991). designing and evaluating early intervention programmes. in d. r. mitchell & r. i. brown, (eds.). early intervention studies for young children with special needs (pp. 297326). london: chapman and hall. moodley, l., louw, b., & hugo, s. r. (2000). early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: a transdisciplinary model of service delivery. the south african journal of communication disorders, 47, 25-39. osofsky, j. d. & thompson, m. d. (2000). adaptive and maladaptive parenting: perspectives on risk and protective factors. in j. p. shonkoff & s. j. meisels (eds.), handbook of early childhood intervention (2nd ed., pp. 54-75). cambridge: cambridge university press. owens, r. e. (2001). language development. an introduction (5th ed.). boston: allyn and bacon. pillay, m., kathard, h., & samuel, m. a. (1997). the c u r r i c u l u m of p r a c t i c e : a c o n c e p t u a l framework for speech-language therapy and audiology practice with a black african first language clientele. the south african journal of communication disorders, 44, 109-117. poggenpoel, m. (1998). data analysis in qualitative research. in a. s. de vos (ed.), research at grass roots. a primer for the caring professions (pp. 334-353). pretoria: j. l. van schaik academic. power, d. (2002, december). opening address. fourth international workshop on kangaroo mother care, cape town, south africa. r o s e n b l i t h , j. f. ( 1 9 9 2 ) . in the beginning: development from conception to age two. newbury park: sage publications. rossetti, l. m. (2001). communication intervention. birth to three (2n d ed.). australia: singular thomson learning. salmon, c. m., rowan, l. e., & mitchell, p. r. (1998). facilitating prelinguistic communication: impact of adult p r o m p t i n g . infant-toddler intervention. the transdisciplinary journal, 8(1), 11-27. samerhoff, a. j., seifer, r., baldwin, α., & baldwin, c. ( 1 9 9 3 ) . s t a b i l i t y of i n t e l l i g e n c e from preschool to adolescence: the influence of social and family risk factors. child development, 64, 80-97. spady, w. g., killen, r., & rand, j. (2000). understanding and applying the saqa critical o u t c o m e s in c u r r i c u l u m p l a n n i n g . l e c t u r e delivered at a symposium, university of south africa, pretoria, south africa. tuomi, s. k. (1994). speech-language pathology in south africa: a profession in transition. american journal of speech-language pathology, may,1 58. 1 turner, t. l., douglas, j., & cockburn, f. (1988). craig's care of the newly born infant (8th ed.), edinburgh: churchill livingstone. 1 van rooyen, e., pullen, a. e., pattinson, r. c.,l& delport, s. d. (2002). the value of the kangaroo m o t h e r c a r e u n i t at k a l a f o n g h o s p i t a l . geneeskunde. the medical journal, april, 6-10;. uys, i. c. (1993). kommunikasiepatologie: onderrig vir die toekoms. the south african journal of communication disorders, 40, 3-9. uys, i. c., & hugo, r. (1997). speech-language pathology and audiology: transformation in teaching, research and service delivery. health sa gesondheid, 2(2), 23-29. wolf, l. s., & glass, r. p. (1992). feeding and swallowing disorders in infancy. tucson:'therapy skill builders. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieeafwykings, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) undergraduate clinical training in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction in sa 13 rt λ ω χι do .3 ώ £ c <2 6 s s cd •ό 9 1 js .52 a, > μ m ο u α ε β <υ υ μ ξ 'β μ β 4» β wi ό wi α> ο f .2 ο » 5 £ « § ^ ^ .2 » e •c ο\ ο ξ s « 5 = s s ·ξ s a β τ a ϋ μ ο β ° 2 •<ο 2 3 3 > " s c ^ ίο 1/3 § i αϊ rj "s s >, ο, 2 ο» μ λ a a j* s ο the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a l t a k r i t z i n g e r and b r e n d a l o u w 1 4 c ® « β 3 β ϊ ·« s 2 § s •s b ό s h e i s ; ε * g 5 * i ο s η s a. ζ v ,q ό t+h 3 ο s β * b ι « !3 •u & 2 e c .2 u w w c ο ω ω > λ a ω β £ \ s -α u s ο g > 2 s β β tts — ο s u h s i s £ · § j2 * ο ΐ ο λ g < ε is 3 s | i j η ιέ 2 ΰ γ ω ~ = si c 's ή — ο ° μ μ μ c 3 ·3 ω.® λ β s s "π c δ i c § δ 1/3 κ e s . s g s c-a oc ^ 9 s" s 5 s , h ω •c ·2 δ ο s i ο ω ε c 3 ^ κ 1 ε2 5 'ξ ο β 3 " & 1 ε i 1 s δ ο -5 -α υ υ ο ο w y £ e a q ο m £ 3 a «> c .3 a g • j cd .2 -5 c 2 w "5? λ " ο m •α on w · sj no ω s s < s i s 3 · ? s3 "> ' 9 •o β ο λ £ ^ 3, ω — ^ % ή -§ ch ο c γw is μ οο ο ο „ os -η -ο ο -η £ ta w ?> « £ .5; (d ο κ t 3 « q ζ m p •sf ο ι 'c bo ι c js cd 00 ί ^ λ ^ c on on c on ο o t ' c (n , ^ s q £ υ (j £,«8 's . 2 3 c .1, 3 ο ο _ β ο «· cd t-< ω s i •β 9 <3 _ • '3 u 3 ω ο β u .ο ω" a °>·3 3 ^ c § ι . ^ (+η £ δ § ° a •§ i 1 s § c3 w u = i · | j | a l i g a g s '£ §) 3 i i | 1 ° δ ° ,ο ο ϋ ϊ g o f i « . •s ω ο «s w · s p i ^ j , ^ „-h s ce < ο s ο • —ι ίλ la i s «2 i ^ ο a 2 cd c λ •-· u -• c 00 ω .3 3 ε -s α ca die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieeafwykings, vol. 50, 2003 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the verbal and nonverbal communicative abilities of pre-adolescent stutterers monica bucher ba (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) transvaal education department psychological and guidance services melissa bortz ba (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) department of speech therapy & audiology coronation hospital, johannesburg denise anderson ba (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract the aim of this study was to investigate and describe some aspects of the verbal and nonverbal communicative competence of six pre-adolescentstutterers. a communication inventory was used to assess competence in relation to four communication functions and two contexts. the sample obtained was transcribed and analyzed in terms of nonverbal and verbal behaviours, including morpheme length and paralinguistic features. the results indicated that the subjects were, on average, incompetent in all areas of communication. correlations were made to stuttering frequency and attitudes to stuttering and sensitivity to communication. diagnostic and therapeutic implications are discussed. uittreksel die doel van die studie was om aspekte van die verbale en nie verbale kommunikasie vermoe van ses pre-adolessente hakkelaars na te vors en te beskryf. 'n kommunikasie inventaris is gebruik om die vermoe in verband met vier kommunikasie funksies en twee kontekste te bepaal. die materiaal is neergeskryf en ontleed in verband met die verbale en nie verbale opdragte, ook met morfeem lengte en parataalkunde kenmerke insluit. die resultate toon dat die proefpersone gemiddeld nie in staat was tot al die aspekte van kommunikasie nie. korrelasies is gemaak tussen hakkel frekwensies en houding en sensitiwiteit vir kommunikasie. diagnostiese en terapeutiese verwysings is bespreek. shames and florance (1980) report on a stutterer who said, "i recall many times that i had been in restaurants and wanted to order something of my choice and anticipated stuttering and would select something else from the menu because it was easier to say that word at that moment" (p. 4). this reveals a marked communication impairment in that it distorts the stutterer's ability to "share" in a social manner, which is what the very word "communication" means (cherry 1978; wood 1981). communication, according to myers and myers (1976) is at the core of our humanness. alant (1979) feels that it expresses social reality. communication may take place by verbal or nonverbal means. verbal communication is defined by wiig and semel (1976) as "the exchange of ideas, intentions, or information by speakers using a common language" (p. 323). abercrombie's (1968) statement that man speaks with his vocal organs, but converses with his whole body, indicates, however, that the mechanical act of speech is only meaningful in its interaction with nonverbal communication. communication does not simply mean "fluent verbal communication" or "adequate nonverbal communication", but rather, effective verbal and nonverbal communication in particular situations (alant 1979). @ sasha 1984 recently the focus of language research has been expanded to include language usage in social contexts. much current research in speech pathology is conducted within a pragmatic functional framework. there is however, a dearth of literature on stuttering within this paradigm. dalton and hardcastle (1977) feel that the major effect of stuttering on communication may perhaps be the sheer absorption that stutterers have in their speech problem. this possibly results in little opportunity to develop adequate communication skills, leading to unspontaneous, inappropriate and insensitive use of language. as barbara (1972) points out, the stutterer's immediate reaction is usually "what have i said? was it clear? were they able to understand me?" nonverbal communication would express this fear to the listener as it is an accurate and spontaneous transmitter of communicating emotions and conveying information about the person (argyle 1975). luper and mulder (1964) describe the most severe communicative impairment of the stutterer as that in which the stutterer refuses to talk for fear of stuttering. stuttering interferes considerably with communication. , muma (1978) states that by the pre-adolescent stage, pragmatic skills should have been acquired. features of communicax the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the verbal and nonverbal communicative abilities of pre-adolescent stutterers 23 tion that children should be familiar with and use by the middle of primary school, include a fluid flow of initiating and responding to ideas, and a simultaneous use of verbal and non verbal means within an interpersonal framework of communication. as regards the latter, it is interesting to note that stuttering has been described as an interpersonal disorder by both sheehan (1968) and van riper (1978). therefore it is important to investigate the pre-adolescent stutterer's use of communication. the present study aims to describe some aspects of the verbal and nonverbal communication performance of a group of pre-adolescent stutterers in order to determine whether they demonstrate inadequate or delayed socialverbal communication skills; and whether stuttering behaviour, attitudes to stuttering and sensitivity to communication are related to communicative competence. methodology subjects (ss) six male pre-adolescent stutterers were used. this represents the smallest sample size possible for non-parametric withingroup studies (saling 1983). description of subjects the subjects were phase iii or iv stutterers (bloodstein 1969) within the nine to ten year old age range. none of the subjects had received previous communication skills training. all were monolingual english speakers. materials 1. communication inventory: the let's talk inventory for adolescents (wiig 1982) was used to assess selective aspects of social-verbal communicative competence of subjects in terms of four communication functions, ritualising, informing, controlling and feeling, in relation to two contexts — peer and adult. 2. attitude to stuttering: j the iowa scale (johnson, darley and spriestersbach 1963) was used for this purpose. 3. sensitivity to communication: alant's personal and social sensitivity scales (1979) were used to assess the subjects' sensitivity to communication. procedure j the ss were assessed individually in the same sound treated room with the same seating arrangements. a video recorder was used to ensure that all ss's responses were accurately recorded for later analysis. the video equipment (national vhs) was placed on the other side of a one-way mirror to make the interaction as natural as possible. testing cpnditions were kept constant to ensure that the environment did not affect subject-examiner interactions. ( the attitude to stuttering and sensitivity to communication scales were administered after the communication inventory, so as to ensure no affect on communication behaviour. the data obtained from the communication inventory was rated by two speech pathologists in terms of a) verbal and b) nonverbal behaviours. the latter rating is an extension of wiig's original procedure, and was devised by the writers. a) verbal communication analysis the verbal behaviours, including paralinguistic features, a morpheme count, communicative competence and related stuttering symptomatology were analyzed in the following way: paralinguistic features'. these were assessed by one of the writers (e) and an independent rater (r) for each s along eight parameters, according to a four point rating scale: 1 — poor; 2 — fair; 3 — adequate; 4 — good. the parameters included rhythm, pitch, intonation, intensity, stress, rate, pausing and embolophrasia. morpheme analysis: the segments were scored to reflect the mean length of utterance (mlu) in morphemes (wiig 1982), for the functions and contexts. this score reflects "syntactic-semantic" complexity. as wiig (1982) pointed out, length does not always reflect verbosity, but may reflect word-finding efforts, circumlocutions, and topical discontinuity. these factors are often very prevalent in stutterers' communication. communication competence: competence was scored as a factor of intent (functions) and register (contexts), and related to the norms established by wiig (1982) and compared as percentages. speech act association scores were also computed to assess the ss' receptive communicative competence. related verbal stuttering behaviours: stuttering behaviours were analyzed in terms of frequency and severity according to a four-point rating scale based upon johnson et al. (1963). the ε and r independently rated the scores for each s on each item of the inventory, according to the following: 0 — no evidence of verbal stuttering behaviour. 1 mild stuttering behaviour, but not interfering with communication. 2 — moderate stuttering behaviour, consisting of a single severe symptom or two mild examples, thus interfering somewhat with verbal communication. 3 — severe stuttering behaviour characterized by two or more examples of severe stuttering, interrupting the smooth flow of verbal communication. b) nonverbal communication analysis the writers devised a nonverbal communication analysis that borrowed from several researchers (argyle 1972; friesen et al. 1979; johnson et al. 1963; knapp 1978; ling and ling 1974). eight movements (see table 1) formed the data-base and were rated according to the following categories by two raters: (i) nonverbal stuttering behaviour (secondary stuttering). (ii) nonverbal behaviour distracting from communication. (iii) nonverbal behaviour complementing communication. the rating consisted of marking down the presence of a movement in the sequence in which it occurred in the "speech act" of "moment" of stuttering" that is, while listening to the stimuli and before, during and after speaking (bales 1960; van riper 1973 and 1978). this would determine how stuttering was related to nonverbal communication and which behaviours detracted from or complemented communication. the speech act or "single interaction" was defined as the "smallest discriminable segdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31. 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 monica bucher, melissa bortz and denise anderson table 1 movements forming the basis for analysis behaviour description of movement head movements these were described as deviations of the head from a midline position, the face facing forward. gaze direction of gaze was considered as looking at partner, looking down or up, or to the side, or at the stimuli. facial expression this included facial configurations which displayed affect. facial extremities were defined as scalp, ears and temples. posture this included any change in general posture as well as varying positions in the trunk and shoulders and lower extremities. it was also the general category under which illustrators and manipulators were considered. illustrators and gestures these were movements which accompanied speech and illustrated what was being said verbally. they may have been present in all parts of the body. shrug this category was defined by the writers and involved a more subtle shoulder movement accompanying speech. manipulators these were either (i) self manipulators which included movements on various parts of the body, for example, finger scratching the palm of the hand, or (ii) object manipulators which were similar actions involving inanimate objects. lower extremities this involved any movement occurring in the upper or lower leg/legs or the ankle. ment of verbal or nonverbal behaviour" (bales 1960, p. 37). for a movement to be considered for analysis, both raters had to have agreed as to its placement within a particular category. the modified iowa scale (johnson et al. 1963) and personalsocial sensitivity scales (alant 1979) were then scored for each s and were related to each other and to communicative competence. statistical procedure the following non-parametric, descriptive statistical procedures used by wiig (1982) to analyze verbal behaviour, were extended, for the present study, to nonverbal behaviour as well. using the raw scores obtained by the group for each communicative function and context, a measure of central tendency (mean), ranges of scores, rankings from highest/best scores to lowest/worst scores and predominant scores, were calculated. inter-rater reliability for the verbal analysis was calculated as a percentage of agreement. results and discussion inter-rater reliability for the verbal analysis was found to be high (88%). communication inventory a) verbal analysis the results revealed that the subjects' verbal communicative abilities were on average ineffective. paralinguistic incompetence was demonstrated in the features of rhythm, pitch, rate and pausing (table 2). as expected with stutterers, rhythm was shown to be generally uneven, although two subjects presented with rhythmical speech patterns as a fluency facilitator. pitch was predominantly monotonous. the speech rate was judged to be slower than normal; pausing prior to speech onset and hesitations during speech production were prevalent in two of the subjects. johnson et al. (1963) suggested that these factors may be due to the fact that the stutterer proceeds apprehensively and cautable 2 overall paralinguistic competence for all ss' expressed as predominant scores, mean scores and rankings feature predominant score mean rank rhythm 2 1,7 7 pitch 1 1,5 8 intonation 3 2,8 4 intensity 4 3,8 1 stress 4 3,5 2 rate 1 1,9 6 pausing 2/3/4 2,7 5 embolophrasia 4 3,2 3 code 4 good 3 adequate 2 fair i 1 poor ; ι tiously as if looking for trouble. the feature of embolophrasia e.g. "er" and "um" and word repetitions was used as part of the stuttering symptomatology by two subjects. the other paralinguistic parameters were found to be largely unaffected by the stuttering. these results confirmed bloodstein's statement in 1969, that what can be identified as stuttering, is not only evident in the fluency impairment, but also in certain paralinguistic parameters and that these features are not always confined to discrete moments. results of the morpheme count (as shown in tables 3 and 4) revealed that the subjects used longer utterances when addressing adults, than when addressing peers. this is in accordance with what wiig (1982) found with normal speakers ̂ however, the subjects generally scored below the norms. they tended to use simple statements, with little elaboration. dalton and hardcastle (1977) described some stutterers, who cut down their utterances to the bare minimum, simplifying an argument to the point they felt they could make easily. one subject used extended utterances. however, this still rendered the commuthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the verbal and nonverbal communicative abilities of pre-adolescent stutterers 25 nication ineffective, mainly as a result of circumlocutions and rambling. the subjects in this study therefore did not appear to have learned the principle expounded by wood (1981) that one must say just enough, but not too much. table 3 morpheme analysis related to the two contexts context mean peer adult 8,6 9,7 table 4 morpheme analysis related to the four functions function predominant score ritualizing informing controlling feeling 1 1 ϊ | above norm same as norm | below norm for verbal competence, both norm relations (wiig 1982) and overall percentages were analysed on the communication inventory. in table 5 it can be seen that, with respect to the contexts, the subjects performed most effectively in relation to the peer context. table 5 verbal competence on inventory: stuttering frequency context ®/o peer adult 79 60 table 6 summary of the mean percentage and predominant scores for ss' communicative com. petence on all fourjcommunication functions wi' thin both contexts function context predominant „, score / 0 ritualizing informing controlling feeling peer, adult peer adult peer adult peer adult ! τ 53 i 83 • 61 1 78 1 62 ί 85 j 62 code | above norm same as norm | below norm wiig (1982) pointed out that this was to be expected in relation to the norms. thus, the stutterers tended to follow the normal trend of effectiveness, although when related to the normative data, they functioned below average for both peer and adult contexts. as can be seen from table 5, the stuttering behaviour was shown to be considerably worse in the adult context. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31 1984 in terms of the functions of communication (table 6), the subjects performed least effectively on the ritualising function, in relation to both contexts. the informing function revealed a considerably better performance in the peer context than in the adult context, although wiig (1982) reported that both contexts should have been acquired. the stuttering severity (tables 7 and 8) appeared to be least severe in relation to the ritualising function and most severe in relation to the informing function. table 7 stuttering analysis using predominant scores related to the four functions function predominant score ritualizing 0 informing 3 controlling 2 feeling 1 code 0 no stuttering 1 mild stuttering 2 moderate stuttering 3 severe stuttering table 8 stuttering analysis using predominant scores related to the two contexts context predominant score peer adult 0;1 2;3 code 0 no stuttering 1 mild stuttering 2 moderate stuttering 3 severe stuttering this may be related to the propositionality level associated with the different functions, since, as eisenson (1975) pointed out, stuttering increases when meaningfulness increases. the controlling function revealed the poorest scores in relation to the norms of all four functions. this too, may be due to the higher propositionality of speech involved (eisenson 1975). the subjects scored most effectively on the feeling function in terms of percentages and in relation to the normative data, yet they still scored below the norm. this may be related to the high personal sensitivity scores of the subjects revealing that they were possibly more in touch with their own feelings. thus the results indicated that the subjects did not perform according to the average standard described by wiig (1982). wiig suggested that this could be related to secondary emotional reactions interfering with the communication learning process. b) nonverbal analysis the subjects did not use nonverbal communication to either enhance or detract from verbal communication, across pragmatic functions. this indicates a delay, as wood (1981) stated that pre-adolescents should have basic pragmatic skills. seconr ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 monica bucher, melissa bortz and denise anderson dary stuttering did vary according to functions when related to propositionality. thus infrequent stuttering was found for ritualising as this function contains little propositionality. an increase in both the amount and severity of secondary stuttering behaviours was found for the informing function as this implies propositionality. the most frequent amounts of stuttering were found for the controlling function, where propositionality is high. this function also requires skill which, according to hopper and naremore (1978), adults often do not acquire. stuttering was decreased for the feeling function. this was possibly because this function was the last to be assessed and the subjects were more relaxed and familiar with the test situation. table 9 summary of the comparison of scores of nonverbal stuttering (st) distracting (d) and complementing (c) behaviours (percentages were calculated relative to the frequency of occurrence of the functions) st % d % c % head movements 4 31 6 46 3 23 gaze 4 22 6 33 8 44 facial expression 5 50 3 30 2 20 posture 4 31 6 46 3 23 gesture and illustrators 3 50 3 50 shrug 3 100 manipulators 3 30 7 70 lower extremities 2 8 9 82 as illustrated in table 9, limited effective communication occurred for head movements. nodding was the only frequent complementing head movement observed, particularly during listening and speaking. this correlated with knapp's (1978) statement that the head nod is probably the most familiar nonverbal act which maintains conversation. distracting head movements and secondary stuttering behaviour also detracted from effective communication. secondary stuttering head movements occurred during speaking as a release from a block as well as before stuttering as a starter. a close relationship between head movements and gaze was noted. eye contact is an important facet of face to face communication which occurs during listening and speaking, just before or after a thought unit is completed (knapp 1978). gaze acts were present on the complementing-checklists indicating adaptive eye contact. however maladaptive gaze patterns were also noted indicating evasion of social contact and interaction. facial expression is the second most commonly occurring stuttering symptom according to bloodstein (1960). it was found in these subjects in the form of lip pursing before and during stuttering, resulting in ineffective communication. high counts of shifting were present before and during speaking making posture one of the most distracting categories of nonverbal behaviours observed. argyle (1977) states that posture is less well controlled than other nonverbal clues and is often an indicator of leakage, that is concealing information from the other interactor. gestures accompanying speech illustrate what is being said verbally (knapp 1978). generally the presence of illustrators was tentative and indicative of ineffective communication. a high percentage of restless or repetitive leg and foot acts formed part of the stuttering pattern. at other times these movements distracted from the communicative process. in summary nonverbal behaviours were detracting from, rather than complementing verbal communication. secondary stuttering exacerbated ineffective communication. attitudes to stuttering the subjects demonstrated an average mean score of attitudes to stuttering on the iowa scale. nevertheless, the predominant scores revealed positive attitudes to stuttering. these positive attitudes did not correlate with the ineffective verbal and nonverbal communication parameters found. this may be a feature of pre-adolescence since the pre-adolescent stutterer has not yet experienced the accompanied social pressures and may therefore not have developed the negative attitudes so common in adults (dalton and hardcastle 1977). sensitivity to communication on the personal social sensitivity scale (alant 1979), the subjects demonstrated high scores on the personal dimension and low scores on the social dimension. the high personal rating probably indicated that the subjects were involved in the encoding function and tended to be selfcentred and absorbed in their stuttering, without considering the other communicators or decoding function. the low social rating probably pointed to the fact that the subjects were not involved adequately in the interaction, revealing their incompetence in their communication and lack of awareness of others. this correlated with the ineffective interactive communication found in this study, and showed that the stutterers did not comply with the prerequisites for high social sensitivity described by alant (1979). conclusions the results of this study revealed that the ss' communication abilities were on average ineffective. this led to ineffective communication and poor social interaction. thus, speech therapy with stutterers of this age group should focus on both fluency and remediation of communication. the ineffective conversation was demonstrated a) within all communication functions, particularly in the controlling function, and in relation to the adult context more than the peer context, b) in the shorter mean morpheme length and simple utterances used, with little elaboration, c) by certain paralinguistic features being of poor quality — uneven rhythm, monotonous pitch, slow rate, pausing before and during utterances, and poor intonation at times. the paralinguistic incompetence could be directly related to the stuttering pattern, while the communicative incompetence in terms of intent and register, and morpheme length may also be related to the stuttering. behaviours complementing communication were present, but in limited amounts. contrary to this, large and varied amounts of distracting nonverbal behaviours were found. these were exacerbated by the nonverbal stuttering actions, which constituted part of the communication problem. the nonverbal skills were not complementing verbal behaviour for pragmatic purposes. pragmatic patterns emerged for secondary stuttering which seemed to relate stuttering to propositionality. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the verbal and nonverbal communicative abilities of pre-adolescent stutterers 27 attitudes to stuttering were shown to be generally positive, which contradicts the hypothesis that poor communication results from poor attitudes. this may be related to the preadolescent age, when social pressure has been minimal. therefore, it seems worthwhile to introduce therapy at this age, as this population seems to have a positive attitude towards communication that is uncharacteristic of older stutterers. however, the low interactive communication sensitivity and poor communication skills indicate that although the ss' were intensely aware and sensitive to their own communication, they had low social sensitivity. in conclusion it must be recognised that therapists must deal with much more than the speech of stutterers in order to give them "the tools needed to help them rejoin the human race" (van riper 1978, p. 6). references abercrombie, d. paralanguage, chapter 2, in laver, j. and hutcheson, s. (eds.): communication in face to face interaction, middlesex, penguin books ltd., 1968. alant, e. speech and language pathology and communication, master of arts, university of pretoria, 1979. argyle, m. bodily communication, london, methuen and co. ltd., 1975. argyle, m. nonverbal communication in human social interaction, in hinde, r. a. (eds.): nonverbal communication, great britain, cambridge university press, 1977. barbara, p.a. communication in stuttering, chapter 22, in emerick, l.l. and hamre, c.e. (eds.): an analysis of stuttering: selected readings, illinois, the interstate printers and publishers inc., 1972. bloodstein, o. a handbook on stuttering, chicago, national easter seal society for crippled children and adults, 1969. bloodstein, o. the development of stuttering i. changes in nine basic features, j. speech hearing dis. 1960, 25, ,219-237. j cherry, c. on human communication: a review, a survey and a criticism, (3rd ed.), england, the mit press, 1978. dalton, p. and hardcastle, e.j. disorders of fluency, london, edward arnold ltd., 19717. eisenson, j. stuttering as a perseverative behaviour. chapter i vi, in, eisenson, j. (ed.): stuttering: a second symposium, new york, harper and row publishers, 1975. erickson, r.l. assessing communication attitudes amongst stutterers, j. speech hear. res. 1969, 12, 711-724. hopper, r. and naremore, r.j. children's speech a practical introduction to communication development, second edition, new york, harper and row publishers inc., 1978. johnson, w., darley, f.l. and spriestersbach, d.l. diagnostic methods in speech pathology, new york, 1963. knapp, m.l. nonverbal communication in human interaction, second edition, new york, holt, rinehart and winston, 1978. luper, h.l. and mulder, r.l. stuttering: therapy for children, englewood cliffs, new jersey, prentice hall inc., 1964. muma, j.p. language handbook concepts, assessment, intervention, englewood cliffs, new jersey, prentice hall inc., 1978. myers, g.e. and myers, m.t. the dynamics of human communication: a laboratory approach. (second edition), mcgraw-hill book company, 1976. saling, m. personal communication, senior lecturer, dept. of psychology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, 1983. shames, g.h. and florance, c.l. stutter-free speech: a goal for therapy, columbus, ohio, charles e. merrill publishing co., 1980. sheehan, j. stuttering as a self-role conflict. chapter v, in, gregory, h.h. (ed.): learning theory and stuttering therapy, evanston, northwestern university press, 1968. shuter, r. understanding misunderstandings, exploring interpersonal communication, new york, harper and row, 1979. van riper, c. speech correction: principles and methods, sixth edition, englewood cliffs, new jersey, prentice hall inc., 1978. wiig, e.h. let's talk inventory for adolescents, manual, columbus ohio, charles e. merrill publishing company, 1982. wiig, e.h. and semel, e.m. language disabilities in children and adolescents, columbus, ohio, charles e. merrill publishing company, 1976. wood, b. children and communication, second edition, englewood cliffs, new jersey, prentice hall inc., 1981. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) siemens quality hearing aids and school teaching equipment from r e p u b l i c h e a r i n g c o n s u l t a n t s ( p t y ) ltd new address: 187 jan smuts avenue lower rosebank (opposite thrupps) telephone: 442-8691 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'ν palatografies-fotografiese ondersoek van die afrikaanse konsonantklanke h.e.c. tesner m.a. (pret.) e.c. naude m.a. (log) (pret.) departement spraakwetenskap, spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria. opsomming die konsonantartikulasie van een afrikaanssprekende proefpersoon is palatografiesfotografies ondersoek om vas te stel wat die ko-artikulatoriese invloede van verskillende vokaalomgewings op die artikulasiekontakpunt van besondere konsonante is. afleidings word gemaak met betrekking tot die stabiliteit van-die artikulasiepunt vir die verskillende konsonante. die verslag word afgesluit met enkele terapeutiese riglyne en aanbevelings vir verdere navorsing. summary consonant articulation of an afrikaans-speaking subject was researched in a palatographicphotographic study in order to establish the co-articulatory effect of different vowel contexts on the place of articulation of specific consonants. deductions are made as to the stability of place of articulation of the different consonants. certain guidelines for therapy are given as well as suggestions for further research. die palatografiese tegniek is een van die oudste eksperimentele hulpmiddels tot die beskikking van die fonetikus wat artikulasie bestudeer. ten spyte hiervan is hierdie tegniek nog nooit uitgebreid toegepas om die invloed van varierende vokaalkontekste op die plasing vir konsonantartikulasie in afrikaans te bestudeer nie. dit word in die fonetiese wetenskap aanvaar dat spraak op artikulatoriese vlak streng gesproke nie segmenteerbaar is nie. nogtans is die praktiese waarde van 'n afsonderlike beskouing van konsonantklanke goed bekend by onderwysers in die fonetiek sowel as by spraakterapeute. meerdere kennis oor die gedrag van die artikulators vir klanke in verskillende omgewings kan ons begrip van artikulasie as 'n dinamiese proses slegs verbreed. hierdie fisiologies-georienteerde kennis moet tesame met 'n linguisties-georienteerde beskouing van die distinktiewe klankeienskappe toegepas word. metode slegs een proefpersoon is by die studie betrek. die palatum is bestuif met 'n mengsel van houtskoolen sjokoladepoeier en na elke artikulasie is 'n kleurfoto van die monddak met 'n "polaroid cu-5 land camera" geneem. die kamera is voorsien van 'n ringflits en 'n mondspieel van verchroomde staal. hibitane is as ontsmettingsmiddel gebruik. die konsonante wat by die studie betrek is en die klankomgewings waarin hulle geproduseer is, verskyn in tabel i. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 h.e.c. tesner & e. naude iti idi ili ini iri i r i isi izi itu u t u uti θ t a idu u d u udi a d o ilu ulu uli a 1 a inu u n u uni a n a u r u uri a r a i r u u r u u r p i j i iqi iki ixi isu usu usi a s a izu uzu uzi a ζ a i j u u j u u j i i p i u j i u u p i it]u u n u uri" iku u k u uki a k a ixu uxu uxi a χ a igu ugu ugi 3 c u ici a g a j a uci t a b e l i. die konsonante wat by die studie betrek is en die klankomgewings waarin hulle geproduseer is. die iru-kombinasie is nie in die tabel verteenwoordig nie, omdat geen bevredigende fotografiese beeld van die produksie verkry kon word nie. verwerking van resultate: elke foto is vir ontledingsdoeleindes onderverdeel soos in fig. 1. met behulp van hierdie verdeling is die volgende metings geneem: 1. afstand van aanraking vanaf voortande. 2. antero-posterior breedte van aanraking. 3. unilaterale area van aanraking links. 4. unilaterale area van aanraking regs. voorbeelde van hierdie metings verskyn in tabelle ii en iii. bostaande metings is uit praktiese oorwegings na grafiekpapier oorgedra wat terselfdertyd 'n duideliker voorstelling van die artikulasiekontak gegee het en dit maklikergemaak het om aanrakingsareas numeries te verwerk. hierdie metode het die kwantitatiewe vergelyking van gegewens vir verskillende klanke moontlik gemaak. (sien grafiek 1). c β α β c 4 5 _ 3 _ 2 1 s p i e e l b e e l d v a n bo t a n d e bo t a n d e figure 1. 1 the south african journal of communication disorders. vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) afrikaanse konsonantklanke 19 iti c β a β c m » u l r grafiek 1 klanki-i i-u u-u u-i θ omgewings t 0-1,50 0-1,75 0-1,50 0-1,67 0-2 r 1,50-2 1,33-1,67 1,50-1,75 1,25-1,50 verklaring: die eerste letterwaarde vir elke klankomgewing dui op die afstand van aanraking vanaf die voortande. die afstand tussen die twee letterwaardes vir elke klankomgewing reflekteer die anteroposterior breedte van die aanraking. raadpleeg fig. 1. tabel 11. afstand van aanraking vanai vooriandc en unteroposierior breedte van aanraking vir [t] en [rj. i i a a links regs i.inks regs 1 a 1 a 1 a 1 a 2 β 2 b,50 2 a 2 a t 3 β,25 3 b,75 3 b,75 3 b,50 t 4 β,50 4 c,50 4 c 4 c 5 c 5 c,50 5 c 5 c,50 6 c 6 c,50 6 c,25 6 c75 1 c,50 1 c,50 1 c,50 1 c,50 2 a 2 a 2 c.50 2 c,50 r 3 b,50 3 b,50 3 a,50 3 a 4 b,25 4 b,50 4 β 4 c 5 b,50 5 c,25 5 c 5 c,50 6 6 6 6 t a b e l 111. laterale aanraking. die standaardafwyking van die area van aanraking vir elke konsonantklank in die verskillende klankomgewings is bereken om η aanduiding te gee van die stabiliteit van die artikulasiekontakpunt. dit word in tabel iv weergegee. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 h.e.c. tesner & . naude gemiddelde standaardafwyking t: 34,8 8,37 γ: 53,6 17,3 k: 9,6 3,9 d: 31,2 7,6 s: 71,6 6,4 x: 14,8 6,9 1: 33,8 9 z: 75,6 11,2 g: 10,8 5,7 n: 42 12,4 j : 60,4 19,7 r: 34,5 5,1 q : 14,8 5,5 tabeliv. m e t i n g s w a a r d e s . bevindings uit die grafieke is vir elke klank bereken watter klankomgewing die artikulasiekontakpunt die verste na voor plaas. (sekere klanke is nie by hierdie berekenings ingesluit nie, t.w. die ruisklanke [s j,[z ] en [ j ] asook [ s 1 en [c]). die klarikomgewings vir elke klank is vervolgens in volgorde van voor na agter geplaas (tabel v). t iti utu uti itu θ t θ d idu idi udi ad udu 1 ili ale ilu uli ulu η unu uni ini θηθ inu r θ γ θ uru iri uri γ 3 γ 3 iru uri iri uru 13 'ζμ a g a uqu iqu uqi k uki aka iki uku iku x ixi θχθ uxu ixu uxi g 'g' 3g3 ugu igu ugi tabel v. volgorde van voor na agter. uit tabel v blyk dit duidelik dat geen konstante patroon na vore tree nie. 'n kwalitatiewe ontleding van die gegewens lei tot die volgende tentatiewe gevolgtrekkings: die klankomgewing [i-i| blyk in die meeste gevalle vir 'n verplasing na voor verantwoordelik te wees. daarna volg die omgewing [s-a]. die omgewing [u-u] verskuif die kontakpunt in die meeste gevalle na agter. hier moet weer daarop gewys word dat uitsonderings voorkom wat die patroon versteur. ' wat die area van laterale kontak betref, is die klankomgewings vir elke klank weereens gerangskik in volgorde van breed na smal (tabel vi). the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) afrikaanse konsonantklanke 21 t iti itu u t u uti a t a d udi idi idu u d u a d a 1 ilu ulu ili a l a uli η ini uni in u u n u a n a r uri iri a r a u r n γ i r u θ γ θ i r i u r i uf u s isi usi a s a usu isu ζ izu uzi izi uzu s z a j i j " i / i "ji u j u a j a q '13" u r j i i r j u u q u a q a k uki iki iku uki a k a x uxi ixi ixu uxu " a x a g igu igi ugi ugu a g a t a b e l vi. volgorde van breed na smal. ook hier is geen konstante patroon uit te wys nie. dit lyk of enige klankomgewing wat [i] bevat, verantwoordelik is vir die breedste laterale aanraking. die smalste laterale aanraking word in die meeste gevalle veroorsaak deur die omgewing [θ-θ], gevolg deur [u-u]. die ondersoek het baie duidelik getoon dat die grootste laterale aanraking voorkom by die frikatiewe [s],[z] en [j], indien die velere klanke uitgesluit word, lyk dit of die ratelklank tesame met die tikklank gekenmerk word deur die kleinste laterale aanraking. dit is ook opvallend dat hierdie twee klanke met besondere klein mediale aanrakings geartikuleer word. interessant van hierdie proefpersoon is dat artikulasiebewegings deurgaans 'n besondere sterk neiging na regs toon. hierdie patroon het egter geen hoorbare invloed op die akoestiese resultaat van die spraakproduksie nie. die laterale neiging is duidelik waarneembaar in die onderste voorbeeldgrafieke van [s]. die stabiliteit van die artikulasiekontakpunt in verskillende klankomgewings toon 'n wye variasie (tabel iv). die velere klanke word hier buite rekening gelaat omdat moontlike verskuiwings van die area van aanraking na agter nie deur hierdie metode opgeteken kan word nie. die volgorde van mees stabiel na minste stabiel verskyn in tabel vii. r s d t 1 ζ η r j" s t a n d a a r d 5,1 6,4 7,6 8,37 9 11,2 12,4 17,3 19,7 tabel vii. volgorde van mees na minste stabiel t.o.v. area van artikulasiekontak. bespreking 'n ernstige tekortkoming van hierdie studie is dat slegs een proefpersoon daarby betrek kon word. die metode is egter duur en omslagtig. ten spyte van hierdie tekortkoming kan daar tog sekere nuttige aanwendings van die bevindings in terapie gemaak word. in terapie vir addentale artikulasie sou dit volgens hierdie gegewens sinvol wees om aanvanklik die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 h.e.c. tesner & . naude konsonant artikulasie in die [u-u] omgewing vas te le, aangesien hierdie omgewing die kontakpunt na agter verskuif. die mate van motoriese vaardigheid en akkurate plasing wat vir die produksie van [r] vereis word, word beklemtoon deur die groot mate van stabiliteit van artikulasiekontakpunt en die besondere klein anteroposterior area van aanraking. in die geval van 'n persoon wat afsluitingsklanke met 'n bree stootbeweging artikuleer (hoewel die akoestiese resultaat vir klanke soos [t] en [d] bevredigend kan wees) sal 'n bevredigende [r] onwaarskynlik wees. die wisselende areas van aanraking vir die verskillende klankomgewings wys net weer op die noodsaaklikheid van 'n program waarin artikulasie van 'n klank in stelselmatig wisselende klankomgewings geoefen word. dit geld by uitstek vir die [n] en die [j'| (die [r], wat ook 'n groot mate van wisseling t.o.v. artikulasiekontakpunt openbaar, is nie 'n foneem in afrikaans nie). 'n interessante waarneming met betrekking tot [n] is dat hierdie klank wat in afrikaans in 'n groot mate onderworpe is aan ko-artikulatoriese bei'nvloeding deur konsonante skynbaar ook in 'n groot mate deur vokale bei'nvloed word. hierdie studie is in vele opsigte 'n ideale onderwerp vir meer gesofistikeerde bestudering met behulp van nuwe elektropalatografiese tegnieke. aangesien hierdie tegniek nog nie in suid-afrika beskikbaar is nie, sal data wat bostaande bevindings verwerp of bevestig, waardevol wees. books on speech and hearing c o n s u l t campus bookshop 34 bertha street, p.o. box 31361 braamfontein 2017 telephone 39-1711 westdene medio books 35/6 nedbank plaza, westdene medio books 175 beatrix street pretoria telephone 2-6336 / logans university bookshop . . 227/9 francois road, durban • telephone 35-4111 overseas publications obtained promptly. nationwide mail-order service. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the ie-30a system from audio analysis ivie spst sushi v i i c e s t e e i c n e 0 u s l y c o m p ' e t e f r e ^ e n c y spectrum of sounds, 2) portable, hand held, rechargeable battery or mains oderated complete w,th precision microphone, batte^charter and c a s n g c a s e 3) two memories, individual store spectrum pattern. 4) may be used to generate voice prints. 5) contains precision sound level meter with digital readout 6) output for oscilliscope, chart recorder, etc. for further information contact: colosseum acoustics 95. kerk street, johannesburg. phone 23-4541 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die meting van middeloorfunksie van die normale menslike oor: 'n timpanometriese ondersoek i . s . h a y , m.a. ( a u d . ) (wash.) departement spraakwetenskap, spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria opsomming nege-en-veertig normaalhorende proefpersone (96 ore) is onderwerp aan 'n timpanometriese ondersoek met 'n grason-stadler otoadmittansmeter. timpanogramme van konduktansie (g) en susseptansie (b) is gelyktydig geregistreer met meetfrekwensie 220 hz en daaina met 660 hz en oorkanaal drukverandering is in 'n positiewe sowel as negatiewe rigting toegepas. sodoende is vir elke oor 8 timpanogramme verkry waarvan die statiese gen b-waardes bereken is. statiese admittansie (y) en impedansie (z) is volgens die standaardmetode bereken asook die fasehoek ( 0 ) . die resultate het getoon dat enige van die gen b-timpanogramme gebruik kan word vir die bepaling van middeloordruk. die rigting van die drukverandering het verskille by indiwiduele ore opgelewer, maar geen betekenisvolle verskille ten opsigte van die gemiddelde waardes nie. die vorm van die timpanogramme bewys dat vals positiewe resultate veral by 660 hz moontlik is aangesien normale ore 'n redelike mate van tipe d-timpanogramme openbaar. die studie gee aanduidings van die moontlikheid dat timpanometrie o.a. by groot positiewe en negatiewe druk in die oorkanaal, inligting kan verskaf aangaande middeloorfunksie. summary forty-nine normal hearing subjects (96 ears) were subjected to a tympanometric investigation using a grason-stadler otoadmittance meter. conductance (g) and susceptance (b) tympanograms were recorded simultaneously using probe-tone frequencies 220 hz and 660 hz. tympanograms were recorded with ear canal pressure variation in a positive and negative direction. this gave a total of eight tympanograms for each ear which were used to obtain static values for g and b. from this data static admittance (y) and static impedance (z) and phase angle ( 0 ) were calculated using the standard formulae. the results indicated that any of the g and β tympanograms could be used for determining middle-ear pressure. the direction of pressure change gave slightly different values of middle-ear pressure for some individual ears but there was no significant difference across subjects. the shape of the tympanograms proved that false positives are possible, especially using the 660 hz probe t o n e , since a considerable number of normal ears displayed type d tympanograms. this research provides indications that tympanometry could furnish information relating to middle-ear function when considering results at great positive and negative pressure levels. sedert metz in 1946 die meting van sekere aspekte van die impedansie op normale en patologiese ore gepubliseer het, het verskeie publikasies van navorsers in europa en die v.s.a. die lig gesien. binne twee jaar na die publikasie in amerika van die kliniese toepassing van timpanometrie in 1970 het timpanometrie 'n baie populere kliniese tegniek geword.4 vandag word die tegniek die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 i. s. hay wereldwyd toegepas en dit word beskou as redelik betroubaar en maklik uitvoerbaar. in die meeste kommersieel-beskikbare apparaat word daar slegs gebruik gemaak. van een aspek van impedansie, nl. die reaktiewe komponent en omdat die oor by 220 hz styfheid beheerd is, word slegs die 'compliance' (omgekeerde van die styfheid) gemeet. die reele komponent van impedansie (akoestiese weerstand) word as so laag beskou vir die menslike oor dat dit nie gemeet word nie. metings deur zwislocki en feldman15 het egter bewys dat waardevolle inligting tog bekom word indien die weerstandskomponent ook gemeet word. hierdie navorsers het egter in 1970 slegs impedansmetings gemaak en dit was eers in 1974 dat feldman3 met behulp van die grason-stadler otoadmittansmeter timpanometrie bestudeer het gebruikmakende van beide reele en reaktiewe komponente van admittansie (die omgekeerde van impedansie) beide by 220 hz en 660 hz. die gebruik van hierdie lae en hoe frekwensies het reeds getoon dat die hoer frekwensies (660 hz of 800 hz) besonder gevoelig is vir oordrom-abnormaliteite. alberti en jerger1 het dit egter duidelik gestel dat geen addisionele inligting bekom word deur by 800 hz te meet nie aangesien die w-tipe timpanogram by feitlik alle soorte middeloorpatologiee voorkom by 800 hz. hulle het egter net 'compliance' gemeet en het dus nie 'n geheelbeeld verkry soos feldman3 nie. die doel van die huidige ondersoek was om (as deel van 'n reeks ondersoeke) die normale menslike oor te onderwerp aan timpanometriese ondersoeke met behulp van die otoadmittansmeter terwyl die konduktansie en susseptansie gelyktydig gemeet word, om die beurt by 220 hz en 660 hz. in 'n tweede fase van die reeks ondersoeke sal soortgelyke metings uitgevoer word op proefpersone met bewese middeloorpatologiee. aangesien daar tot dusver slegs twee publikasies3.5 in hierdie verband verskyn het, word hierdie navorsing as belangrik beskou. metode p r o e f p e r s o n e as proefpersone het opgetree studente en personeel van die departement spraakwetenskap, spraakheelkunde en oudiologie van die universiteit van pretoria. proefpersone is geselekteer op grond van normale gehoor (gehoorpeil nie groter nie as 15 db i s ο by 250 tot 4 000 hz) en normaal verklaar deur 'n otoloog. nege-en-veertig proefpersone (96 ore) is gebruik. die ouderdomme het gewissel tussen 18 en 33 jaar met 'n gemiddelde ouderdom van 20 jaar 10 maande. a p p a r a a t vir die meting van die twee komponente van die admittansie is gebruik gemaak van die grason-stadler otoadmittansmeter model 1720. twee hewlettpackard tipe 7035b x — y registreerders is respektiewelik gekoppel aan die uitgange van die otoadmittansmeter sodat die twee komponente van die admittansie gelyktydig gemeet kon word. horisontale beweging op die registreerthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) timpanometriese ondersoek van middeloorfunksie 63 ders is bewerkstellig deur hulle te koppel aan die otoadmittansmeter se lugdruksisteemse uitgangwatvirhierdie doel beskikbaar is. sodoende is beweging na regs verteenwoordigend van druktoename in 'n positiewe rigting en na links van druktoename in 'n negatiewe rigting. yking van alle apparaat het daagliks geskied volgens voorskrifte van die vervaardigers. korreksies vir hoogte bo seespieel en temperatuur is aangebring op alle admittansiewaardes verkry. o n d e r s o e k m e t o d e metings met die otoadmittansmeter het 8 timpanogramme opgelewer. vir elke oor wat getoets is, is 'n timpanogram vir akoestiese konduktansie, g, en akoestiese susseptansie gelyktydig verkry by beide 220 hz en 660 hz, beide in die positiewe en negatiewe rigting van druk verandering. metings is in beide rigtings uitgevoer ten einde vas te stel of daar betekenisvolle verskille na vore kom wat die vorm van die timpanogramme betref. statiese waardes van g en figure 1. verspreiding van middeloordruk as 'n funksie van meetfrekwensie en admittanskomponente g en b. gg dui aan timpanogram drukverandering in rigting -300 na + 300 mm water §§ dui aan drukverandering in rigting +300 na-300 mm water. g: konduktansie. b: susseptansie. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 i. s. hay β is gebruik vir die berekening van die komplekse admittansie y deur middel van die formule y2 = g 2 + b 2 . die akoestiese impedansie, z, is bepaal deur die formule ζ te gebruik. die fasehoek, 0 , is bepaal deur die formule 0 = tan te gebruik. resultate middeloordruk die verspreiding van die resultate van middeloordruk (die drukwaarde waar die timpanogram se piek voorkom) word vir die linkeren regteroor vir al die meettoestande en rigting van drukverandering op fig. 1 vooigestel. vir al die toestande het meer as 80% van die metings tussen 2 0 en +20 mm waterdruk geval. gemiddelde middeloordrukwaardes vir die verskillende meettoestande en rigting van drukverandering word in tabel i aangegee. die laaste kolomme.gee die algehele gemiddeldes en hiervan kan afgelei word dat die gemiddeldes vir beide ore feitlik 0 mm waterdruk is. die feit dat die linkeroor se gemiddelde drukwaarde positief is en die regteroor s'n negatief, is hier van minder belang omdat beide waardes so klein is. rigting van drukverandering negatief na positief positief na negatief gemiddeld rigting van drukverandering negatief na positief positief na negatief gemiddeld linkeroor (n = 47) rigting van drukverandering negatief na positief positief na negatief gemiddeld g 220 hz 4 , 9 1 ( 5 9 / + 2 4 ) 8 , 3 6 ( s 3 / + 2 3 ) 6 , 6 4 ( 5 6 / + 2 0 , 5 ) g 660 hz 1,14 ( 4 6 / + 2 7 ) 0,17 ( 5 4 / + 3 0 ) 0,66 ( 5 0 / + 2 7 ) β 220 hz 0,70 ( 5 9 / + 2 5 ) 0,87 ( 5 2 / + 2 5) 0,79 ( 5 5 . s / + 2 5 ) β 660 hz 0,34 ( 4 6 / + 50) 0,34 ( 5 3 / + 4 7 ) 0,34 ( 4 9 , 5 / + 3 0 , 5 ) gemiddeld 0 , 6 8 ( 5 9 / + 5 0 ) 1 , 7 5 (—54/+47) 1 , 2 1 5 ( 5 6 / + 3 0 . 5 ) negatief na positief positief na negatief gemiddeld regteroor (n = 49) negatief na positief positief na negatief gemiddeld 7 , 9 3 ( 1 0 0 / + 1 9 ) 1 1 , 4 9 ( 1 0 0 / + 20) 9 , 7 1 ( 1 0 0 / + 1 7 ) 2 , 2 4 (—100/+29) 3 , 7 9 (—103/+36) 3 , 0 2 ( 1 0 1 . 5 / + 2 5 ) 3 , 4 2 (—100/+33) 5 , 8 8 (—100/+27) 4 , 6 5 (—100/+30) 6,71 ( 1 0 0 / + 5 0 ) / / 4,57 ( 9 4 / + 4 6 ) 5,64 ( 9 7 / + 4 8 ) 1 , 7 2 (—100/+50) 4 , 1 5 (—103/+46) 2 , 9 4 (—101,5/+ 48) tabel i gemiddelde middeloordrukwaardes vir timpanogram-optekening van g 220 hz, g 660 hz, β 220 hz en β 660 hz vir drukverandering in beide rigtings. die verspreiding word in hakies aangegee. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) timpanometriese ondersoek van middeloorfunksie 65 die rigting van drukverandering het skynbaar 'n geringe invloed op die middeloordrukwaardes uitgeoefen. in alle gevalle is die gemiddelde waarde 'n meer negatiewe of minder positiewe waarde as die rigting van drukverandering van positief na negatief was en andersom, vir die teenoorgestelde rigting van drukverandering. vir indiwiduele proefpersone was dit nie altyd die geval nie. hierdie verskille is egter so klein dat dit nie kliniese waarde kan he nie. die groter verspreiding van die regteroor (fig. 1 en tabel i ) is die gevolg van relatief groot negatiewe middeloordrukwaardes van twee proefpersone, nl. 1 0 0 mm-waterdruk vir proefpersoon 32 en 8 0 mm-waterdruk vir proefpersoon 40. beide proefpersone se oudiogramme was binne normale perke. jerger6 beskou die normale perke as ± 50 mm-waterdruk terwyl bluestone (soos gemeld in harford4 ) gevind het dat 5 0 mm-waterdruk kan dui op 'n abnormale middeloor. s t a t i e s e a d m i t t an se n i m p e d a n s w a a r d e s statiese konduktansie, g, en susseptansie, b, van die middeloor is verkry deur die verskil te bereken van die waardes vir die middeloor in die gespanne (+ of 3 0 0 mm-waterdruk) en die ontspanne (drukwaar timpanogram 'npiekvorm) toestande. indien die gen b-waardes by + en 3 0 0 mm-waterdruk verskil het, is die gemiddelde van die twee waardes geneem. die hoogte van die piek is dus die statiese waarde.13 daar bestaan 'n verskil van opinie oor wysheid van hierdie metode van meting en daar is navorsers wat verkies om die hoogte van die timpanogram te bepaal by atmosferiese druk aangesien hulle voel dat dit die waarde van g en β bepaal wat in die praktyk geld.7 by die meting van hierdie waardes op proefpersone met normale gehoor, soos in"die huidige studie, is daar nie 'n groot verskil tussen die twee metings, nl. by die maksimum van die timpanogram of by atmosferiese druk nie. die gemiddelde waardes van g, β, υ, ζ en 0 word in tabel ii angegee. resultate gepubliseer deur feldman3 en jacobson et al.s word in tabel iii saamgevat. 'n vergelyking van hierdie resultate en die van die huidige ondersoek toon dat laasgenoemde resultate heelwat hoer is. by nadere ondersoek blyk dit dat' die metode wat feldman3 gebruik het om die statiese gen b-waardes te meet, verskil van die van die huidige ondersoek se metode. feldman het die gen b-waardes by atmosferiese druk gemeet en nie by die piek van die timpanogram soos by hierdie ondersoek die geval was nie. die verskil is veral opmerklik by 660 hz. 'n groter ooreenkoms word egter gevind indien die impedans-waardes van die huidige ondersoek vergelyk word met die resultate van zwislocki en feldman soos gemeld in lilly10 en die van feldman3 soos aangedui in tabel iv. die ooreenstemming is hier merkwaardig. die relatief klein verskil in meetfrekwensie kan geignoreer word. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) i. s. hay /̂ s 00 /"s 00 νο m /"s ·> ν κ <3\ <3\ co t> ο, rr> α ιη co in f ν κ ο 00 γm ^ θ\ τ 1 so <3\ ii ο ν ο i in ι ο ι in i so <3\ ii so so cs 00 •ψ. -η γ<"1 co co m vo. ο" ο" 85 ο„ 00 £ ο ο" co m vo. ο" ο ' χ, t3 s i3 s i3 in 13 νο co 00 νο in ε 1 ο ο 00 γο" ε ν νο „ ο , ο σν ορ ri • ω ο ε ο 7 ο νο 00 «7 σν ορ ri • ω ο ο co i 00 i 00 i ^ i οο" i ° £ ο" 8 " ιη <3\ i ο <ν 1 ιη ο" ct ο" ο" m i 00 m ο • 00 • νο 00 m ο • cs <3\ <3\ 00 m ο • νο" ν ε 3 °ί> • £ s m σ\ -ο τ 1 -3 7, 3 ,6 — — 6ι <3\ ο i νο i i in i -3 7, 3 ,6 — — 6ι <3\ vo vo ο co ci m νο. -3 7, 3 ,6 — — 6ι ii •ψ. γ ί ci m νο. ο. 00 ζ ο" ct ο" ο i t-l ο 2 γο νο co 00 <3\ γο •<-» ο ο 00 00 σ\" οβ ο · pc ν νο ο γο οβ ο · pc ε § 7 3 7 i ο -χ ri 00 i in i 00 i cs ο 2 ο {g ο s " ιη " σ\. vo i co 1 in ο ' ο ο" ο in q ο •ψ νο 00 σ\ τ}· in ν νο γο in ε £ οο σ\ 0 0 vo τ 1 q ι ο ri m i | ^ i co i co 1 1 ^ rvo vo (sf _γ ιη ο" ^ co i co 1 1 ^ ii νο. co_ 00 £ lh ct ct ο" ο ο t-| in υ σ\ ο νο in λ ιη <3\ ro" •s j ν ^ r. 00 ο ^ ο 00 •s j ε £ 9 ιη cs 1 § ρ-, ιη ^ 1 σ\ ι vo 1 1 ο 00 i 00 i ^ i ^ 1 σ\ ι vo 1 1 ο ρ: σ\ ^ 1 σ\ ι vo 1 1 ο <ν ο ο" ό ο ' ο vo 1 ο ε ο ε̂ ο ε 1 i ο υ 13 μ t-l 00 ο cq * ν ό <υ •o c λ > -sl μ α, ω s3 3 3 u <α .. &0 <υ c c α c3 ce +j υ ι * c ο r ί ι ! £ 2 00 ce c c '-β λ 5 g ιλ ο, > .a q ό, 2 * ω ο ' 13 ο 2 « § 2 ο ε j ω « < η the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) timpanometriese ondersoek van middeloorfunksie 67 jacobsonet al. s ( n = 6 0 ) f e l d m a n 3 (n=100) hay (n =96) 220 hz 660 hz 220 hz 660 hz 220 hz 660 hz g (mmho) 0,28 1,7 0,15 1,95 0,301 2,602 β (mmho) 0,58 0,83 0,50 1,30 0,803 1,573 tabel iii gemiddelde waardes van konduktansie, g, en susseptansie, b, vir normale ore impedansie (akoest. ohm) frekwensie zwislocki & feldmanls (n = 66) feldman3 (n = 100) hay (n = 96) 220/250 hz 660/750 hz 1530/—74,1° 521/—41,6° 1856/—69,5° 409/—31,0° 1482/—68,8° 439/—35,2 tabel iv gemiddelde impedanswaardes vir normale ore die vorm van die timpanogram afleidings word van die vorm van die timpanogram gemaak met die oog op die diagnose van middeloorpatologie. in hierdie ondersoek is slegs normaalhorendes gebruik om te poog om normatiewe waardes vas te stel. die variasie wat die vorm van die timpanogram betref, is egter so groot dat feitlik alle vorms wat by verskillende patologiee voorgekom het, waargeneem is. hiervan kan afgelei word dat daar ook vir die normale oor toestande bestaan wat timpanogramme lewer met vorms wat foutiewe diagnoses moontlik maak. tabel v gee die klassifikasie (volgens jerger,6 liden et al.8> 9 ) van die timpanogramvorms wat in hierdie studie verkry is. die getalle dui op die aantal ore wat binne elke kategorie val. die resultate dui daarop dat vir beide g en β by 220 hz byna 80% van die timpanogramme 'n a-konfigurasie het. verhoging van die meetfrekwensie na 660 hz gee vir g 'n toename in die aantal ore wat timpanogramme van die ap-konfigurasie het, d.w.s. 'n meer gedempte sisteem. die effek van frekwensieverhoging op b-timpanogramme, is die toename in die aantal ore met den e-konfigurasie. hierdie konfigurasie word verklaar deur liden et al.9 deur te verwys na die feit dat 660 hz nader aan die resonansfrekwensie van die middeloor is en dat die susseptansie daar hoog is sodat met die oorgang van positiewe na negatiewe druk, die trommelvlies vmmg heen en weer beweeg. 'n ander siening is die van harford,4 nl. dat die d-konfigurasie moontlik die gevolg is van 'n losgekoppelde incudostapediale gewng sodat verandering in die kanaaldruk om en by atmosferiese druk tot gevolg kan he 'n gedeeltelike ontkoppeling van die benige ketting. liden et al.9 het gevind dat 8% van die 182 proefpersone met normale gehoor, tipe d-timpanogramme gelewer het, en hierdie resultaat word bevestig deur hierdie ondersoek (kyk tabel v β 220 hz). vir g 660 hz en β 660 hz word tipe d-timpanogram in 13% en 26% respektiewelik verkry. liden.et a l 9 verbind tipe d met littekens op die trommelvlies. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 i. s. hay — » ο ιο ο ο fs \ο η ^ ιο •ί· ιο ο rs 00 fs ν /-ν os κ ο vo vo ο ο 46 47 32 28 ο ο rs ό σ> ο ii ζ βί χ co ιο 00 69 73 m ο ο •τ 00 vo ο j ν s ο rs fs ο t̂ os t̂ ο ο •τ ο ο \ο ο 00 ο ο cn ο ο η ο 00 rs νο ο rs ο ιο ν s ο fs fs 00 ο fs ίιο /-ν os •τ ο vo ο rs fs m ο fs ο ο ii ζ \ο •ί· fs ιο ο fs •τ ο μ ο μ υ η fs \o m νο ο 1· ο χ ν s •τ t̂ m ο rs ο ο rs fs ο rs fs ο m ο f̂ l ο ο fs fs 1· ιο ο fs ο ο η ο m 1· ο ν s 1 m fs ο ο •τ rs ο «ο /-ν rο m fs ο ιο ο ζ •τ rs fs ό ο ο ιο ο ο μ ο .5 1-1 ο ο μ ο .5 1-1 η m m ο •τ ο ο ο μ ο .5 1-1 ν ε ο 1· fs m ' νο ο ιο ο rs rs rs rs ο 1· ο •τ m ο ο ο•τ 00 ιο ο 1 ο ο1 t ip e t im pa no gr . vi < < «ρ η u q ω < .s s ό j ο > c < •u •o 3 = g-s ^ t-h u ω 2 « ό ' s ε ε ε .2 ι ξ π rt υ λ ι ε + υ s υ <υ > ^ > crt •ο s rt υ 2 ό ό 'ε ο αο 3 •s > ab c ο .a s 2 ό ό ι e = 1 5 rt (λ s s a 5 «1 γλ ω 3 — 60 ia 1-2 ε π c „ c e ta η u * c 3 2 -s c .2 ο o s u ο ο λ (τ) (τ) ε u .· • ~ o. a s i a a ό rt ε λ ό u, top ed ο ο > ο e c .5 .ire ο s rt o. .§ η s o. u g ο η ε·5 υ « "2 ε α s 3 ε 5 ε ω ^ ολ ·° .s -a s s μ μ ο © ο ο ο ο ε .ε c c ο ό co (q μ > > 60 60 ω ω .5 .2 ' f t f t s s c c ό ό ω ω c c 0/5 (λ α α δ μ 3 ^ — hh fch . s s q q < < < β u q ω μ <υ οο <υ , ό c <υ > <υ •3 τλ c <υ ,ορ ~ο > ο > ε η 1-4 οο ο c η α, <υ •3 α η £ η α ζ <υ 1-4 ο α α < > η-ι η η < η r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) timpanometriese ondersoek van middeloorfunksie 69 die implikasies van bogenoemde resultate, is dat foutiewe diagnoses gemaak kan word deur tipe d-timpanogramme te koppel aan dislokasie van die benige ketting, veral by 'n meetfrekwensie van 660 hz. hierdie feit word verder gestaaf deur die werk van van huyse et al. 1 4 wat 'n wiskundige verklaring voorle vir die mof w-patroon. volgens hulle kom die patroon voor wanneer daar die regte kombinasie van druk, admittansie en frekwensie bestaan: timpanogram tipe ag kom wel voor by alle toestande, maar veral by g 220 hz, β 220 hz en g 660 hz. slegs een oor het 'n tipe a-timpanogram gegee by β 660 hz, maar daar moet gemeld w ord dat by hierdie toestand ongeveer 26 ore tipe d gegee het waaronder ingesluit was timpanogramme met pieke wat vergelykbaar was met tipe ag. timpanogram tipe β is nerens waargeneem nie, hoewel heelwat van die aqtipes timpanogramme baie plat verloop het met 'n geringe piek of hoogtepunt. timpanogram tipe c het wel in enkele gevalle voorgekom (tabel v) en in een geval met 'n middeloordrukwaarde van minder as 1 0 0 mm h 2 o. hoewel al die ore deur 'n onk-arts as normaal gesertifiseer is, kan daar tog 'n paar ore gewees het met 'n geringe disfunksie van die buis van eustachius. ook wat tipe ε betref, is enkele ore gevind met hierdie tipe timpanogram veral vir toestand β 660 hz. hierdie tipe word deur liden et al.9 gekoppel aan dislokasie van die benige ketting. die afleiding kan gemaak word dat timpanogramme van g 660 hz en β 660 hz meer vals positiewe resultate gaan oplewer as g 220 hz en β 220 hz 4 die timpanogram kan dus nie sonder meer gebruik word om middeloorpatologie aan te dui nie. beskouing van die korrelasie tussen die timpanogramtipe en die uiterste gevalle van absolute admittanswaarde (tabel vi) gee 'n duidelike beeld van 'n verband tussen hierdie waarnemings. die proefpersone met lae admittanswaardes het feitlik deurgaans 'n tipe a2"(gedempte) timpanogram gegee, terwyl diegene met 'n hoe admittansie 'n tipe d-timpanogram opgelewer het. hierdie korrelasie bestaan ook vir die konduktansieen susseptansie-waardes. timpanogram-tipe admittansie (220 hz) konduktansie (220 hz) susseptansie (220 hz) proefpersoon nr. (linkeroor) ( x = ,860 ) ( x = ,27) ( x = ,74) 9 a2 ,468 ,17 ,39 11 a2 ,404 ,14 ,34 13 • a 2 ,503 ,20 ,44 42 a 2 ,232 ,07 ,20 35 a2 ,382 ,11 ,33 7 d 1,768 ,43 1,55 15 d 1,571 ,64 1,28 30 d 1,785 ,60 1,51 37 d 2,020 ,61 1,72 41 d 1,817 ,69 1,51 tabel vi timpanogram-tipe en hoogste en laagste admittanswaardes by 220 hz die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 i. s hay 'n verskynsel wat opmerklik was, was die verskil wat daar soms bestaan het, tussen die g-sowel as b-waardes by 'n druk van —300 mm en + 300 mm h2 0 . dit word veronderstel dat beide hierdie druktoestande 'n harde wandholte skep wat 'n meting is van die ekwivalente volume gelyk aan die volume van die eksterne meatus met uitskakeling van die middeloor.4 die rigting van die drukverandering het in die meeste gevalle nie verskil gemaak wat die gen b-waardes by 3 0 0 en + 300 mm h2 o-druk betref nie. die verwagting was dat 'n positiewe druk in die oorkanaal groter gof b-waardes tot gevolg sou he aangesien die trommelvlies na binne geforseer word, maar dit was nie altyd die geval nie. tabel vii gee 'n aanduiding van die aantal ore waar die verandering in g en β met druk kleiner was by —300 mm as by + 300 mm water. hieruit kan afgelei word dat vir g 660 hz was die verandering in g in 11% van die ore kleiner vir 'n druk van —300 mm as vir +300 mm-waterdruk in die oorkanaal. a a n t a l o r e g 220 hz β 220 hz g 660 hz β 660 hz regteroor 1 6 42 1 linkeroor 6 1 35 0 totaal 7 ' 7 77 1 tabel vii aantal ore waarvan die verandering in g en β kleiner was by 'n oorkanaaldruk van 3 0 0 mm as by 'n druk van + 300 mmwaterdruk by g 220, β 220 en β 660 hz is daar slegs enkele ore gevind wat genoemde verskynsel openbaar, met ander woorde, in die meeste ore is die omgekeerde waar, nl. dat 'n groot positiewe druk in die oorkanaal 'n kleiner verandering in g en β gee as 'n groot negatiewe druk. hierdie resultaat stem in 'n mate ooreen met metings wat deur mcpherson et al.1 1 op marmotte uitgevoer is. meting van middeloorimpedansie met veranderde middeloordruk, het getoon dat by 200 hz en +300 mm-waterdruk in die middeloor, die impedansverandering groter is as by —300 mm-waterdruk. (dit moet onthou word dat +300 mmwaterdruk in die middeloor soos deur mcpherson gebruik, ooreenstem met +300 mm-waterdruk in die oorkanaal). mcpherson et al. 1 1 se resultaat by 200 hz stem ooreen met die resultaat van hierdie ondersoek by 220ήζ. hul resultaat by 600 hz gee dieselfde verandering in impedansie by +300 mmen —300 mm-waterdruk in die middeloor, terwyl daar in hierdie ondersoek gevind is dat vir g 660 hz, die verandering in admittansie groter is (vir 80% van die ore) vir +300 mm-waterdruk in die oorkanaal as vir 3 0 0 mm-waterdruk. hierdie toestand kom vir die marmot (volgens mcpherson et a l . 1 1 ) alleen voor by hoe frekwensies, bv. by ιοκηζ. j die verskille by die maksimum positiewe en; negatiewe druk, het waarskynlik te make met die verandering in die meganiese eienskappe van die middeloor the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) timpanometriese ondersoek van middeloorfunksie 71 wanneer die stelsel ver na binne of ver na buite geforseer word deur hoe drukwaardes.11 'n groot negatiewe druk in die eksterne oorkanaal het tot gevolg die gedeeltelike ontkoppeling van die incudostapediale gewrig (dus groter wrywing en kleiner g) tesame met 'n groter gespannenheid van die trommelvlies en membraan van die ovale venster. 'n groot positiewe druk in die eksterne oorkanaal vergroot die koppeling van die gewrig (groter g) en vergroot die gespannenheid van die membrane met 'n verdere moontlikheid dat die wyse van beweging van die stapes verander soos met groot klankdruk.2 gevolgtrekkings die doel van die studie was om 'n waardebepaling te maak van die waardes van konduktansie, g, en susseptansie, b, soos gemeet op proefpersone met gehoor binne normale perke. verder moes daar vasgestel word of die rigting van drukverandering by die bepaling van die timpanogram van belang was en of die gebruik van 220 hz sowel as 660 hz as meetfrekwensie van waarde is by die bepaling van normale waardes. die volgende gevolgtrekkings kan gemaak word: die g, β, υ, ζ en 0 waardes in hierdie studie verkry stem in 'n groot mate ooreen met die verkry in ander studies,3'5 > 1 5 hoewel daar toegelaat moet word vir die feit dat die piek op die timpanogram waar g en β gemeet is, verskil van hierdie studie. wanneer daar by die piek gemeet word (soos in hierdie studie) is die waardes van g en β en dus y groter as wanneer die meting by atmosferiese druk gemaak word soos in die studies van feldman3 en jacobson et al.5 die meettoestand (g 220, g 660, β 220, β 660) en rigting van drukverandering in die oorkanaal het min invloed op die meting van middeloordruk. daar was wel 'n opmerklike verskil by indiwiduele proefpersone, maar nie sodanig dat dit diagnostics van betekenis sou wees nie. dit is miskien te verwagte dat 'n meganiese stelsel soos die middeloor effens gevoelig sal wees vir die rigting van drukverandering in die oorkanaal veral by die drukwaardes waar optimum beweeglikheid verkry word. ook by die bepaling van g en β het die rigting van die drukverandering in die oorkanaal geen noemenswaardige invloed gehad nie. die vorm van die timpanogram wat aanduidend is van die beweeglikheid van die middeloor bevat by die normale oor tipes van feitlik al die tipes wat verteenwoordigend is van sekere middeloorpatologiee. die gebruik van die vorm van die timpanogram vir diagnostiese doeleindes moet dus baie versigtig geskied. feldman3 waarsku ook hierteen en meld dat 'n trommelvlies met geneesde perforasies 'n beeld kan gee van 'n baie beweeglike sisteem wat die teenwoordigheid van otosklerose kan verbloem. die gebruik van beide die 220 hz en die 660 hz meettoon is veral van waarde wat die vorm van die timpanogram betref. ookhier moet egter 'n waarskuwing gerig word wat die gebruik van hierdie inligting vir diagnostiese doeleindes betref aangesien die voorkoms van tipe d (mof w-patroon) by proefpersone met normale gehoor redelik aansienlik is (26% van 100 normale ore by β 660 hz die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 i. s. hay en 13% by g 660). die toestand g 220 hz blyk meer betroubaar te wees omdat dit die hoogste persentasie (84%) as tipe a gediagnoseer het en die laagste persentasie (0%) as tipe d. die toestand β 660 hz het die laagste persentasie (56%) as tipe a gediagnoseer en die hoogste persentasie (26%) as tipe d. hieruit kan afgelei word dat daar wel met voordeel van die gebruik van gtimpanogramme gebruik gemaak kan word. die timpanogramme van g 220 hz, β 220 hz, g 660 hz en β 660 hz gee gesamentlik 'n beeld van die middeloorfunksie wat nie moontlik is met slegs β 220 hz en β 660 hz timpanogramme wat gewoonlik met kommersieel-beskikbare vapparaat geregistreer word nie. afleidings wat gemaak kan word van die gen b-waardes met die oordrom in die gespanne toestand (±300 mmwaterdruk) kan moontlik van diagnostiese waarde wees, maar hierdie aspek moet verder ondersoek word. verwysings 1. alberti, p. w. r„ jerger, j. (1974): probe tone frequency and the diagnostic value of tympanometry. arch. otolaryngol., 99, 3, 206-210. 2. bekesy, g. von, (1960): experiments in hearing. mcgraw hill, new york. 3. feldman, a. s., (1974): eardrum abnormality and the measurement of middle ear function. arch otolaryngol., 99, 211-217. 4. harford, e. r„ (1975): tympanometry. in handbook of clinical impedance, jerge.\ j. (ed.). american medics corp., new york. 5. jacobson, j. t., kimmel, b. l., en fausti, s. α., (1975): clinical application of the grason-stadler oto-admittance meter. asha, 1, 11-16. 6. jerger, j. (1970): clinical experience with impedance audiometry. arch. otolaryngol., 92, 311-324. 7. jerger, j. (1976): suggested nomenclature for impedance audiometry. in selected readings in impedance audiometry. northern, j. l. (ed.). american medics corp., new york. 8. lid£n, g., petersen, j. l., bjorkman, g. (1970): tympanometry.arch. otolaryngol., 92, 248-257. 9. liden, g., harford, e., hallen, o. (1974): automatic audiometry in clinical practice. audiology, 13,126-139. 10. lilly, d. j. (1972): acoustic impedance at the tympanic membrane. in handbook of clinical audiology. katz, j. (ed.). baltimore, williams & wilkins co. 11. mcpherson, d. l., miller, j. m., axelsson, a. (1976): middle ear pressure: effects on the auditory periphery./. acoust. soc. amer., 59, 135-142. 12. miller, j. m., holmquist, j. (1974): an animal model for study of eustachian tube and middle ear function.scarttl audiol., 3, 63-68. 13. northern, j. l. (1975): clinical measurement procedures. in handbook of clinical impedance audiometry. jerger, j. (ed.). american medics corp., new york. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, j 977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) timpanometriese ondersoek van middeloorfunksie i 73 14. van huyse, v. j., creten, w. l., van kamp, k. j. (1975): on the w-notching of tympanograms. scand. audiol., 4,45-50. 15. zwislocki, j., feldman, a. s. (1970): acoustic impedance of pathological ears. amer. sp. hear. assoc., monogr. no. 15. our rang ε ft service m e y m e q i m i e i train-ears audiometers hearing aids sound-proof rooms all speech therapy requirements for meedilek w e s t i e m e ( m m s o t m fftvj) l t i telephones: 45-7262 45-6113/4 telegrams: "needlerorg" telejt: 83660 needier westdene mouse 33 durham street raedene johannesburg die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24,1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) f@r © the development of perception ® the acquisition of speech and language skills ® the improvement of motor co-ordination ® helpful texts for therapists © educational toys, books and equipment φ records for auditory training ® catalogues on request © large variety of tests available ply ο play an κ 8 tyrwhitt avenue, rosebank (adjoining the constantia cinema) telephones: 788-1304 p.o. box 52137, saxonwold, tvl. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 dysphagia evaluation and management: clinical training, clinical competency and speciality recognition* bonnie j.w. martin** programme director: the evelyn trammel voice and swallowing center director : rehabilitation services department, saint joseph's hospital of atlanta atlanta, georgia, united states of america key words : dysphagia, swallowing disorders, speech-language pathologist, training, intervention an increasing number of speech-language pathologists have become involved in the evaluation and management of patients with swallowing disorders. approximately sixty percent of practicing speech-language pathologists in health care settings throughout the united states are involved in dysphagia intervention according to the 1993 asha omnibus survey (asha ,1995). it has been estimated that nearly 15 million americans suffer from disorders of deglutition that potentially alter their quality of life, rehabilitation potential and survival (simmons, 1986). in addition to increased patient demand impacting on the growing number of clinicians involved with dysphagic patients, consumers (e.g., patients, families, physicians, etc.) have also begun to recognize dysphagia management as a clinical science and the value added patient care service. the majority of dysphagic patients seen in hospitals and skilled nursing facilities have concomitant communication problems that may include disorders of voice, motor speech, language or cognition (martin & corlew, 1990). from a continuity of care arid cost perspective it follows that the speech pathologists! traditionally trained in the function of the neurologic system and vocal tract, also treat the functionally impaired upper aerodigestive tract comprised of structures common to the communication process (martin & corlew, 1990)1 however, speech-language pathologists have met with several challenges in their attempts toward dysphagia intervention with the often medically complex, multisystem involved patient. these challenges include the following: 1. inadequate educational and clinical preparation at the undergraduate and graduate level; 2. lack of methods for completing and measuring clinical competency in the areas of dysphagia management; 3 special patient populations (i.e., pediatric, ventilator dependent, head and neck surgical, tracheotomized) warrant acquisition of specific skills obtained in facilities not available to many student clinicians; 4 most employment opportunities in medical settings require dysphagia training and experience. because only a handful of accredited university programs in the united states offer courses in swallowing function and disorders, clinicians have sought other training alternatives that include conferences and workshops presented by colleagues who have self-acquired clinical experiential expertise, journals and books, or through observation of practicing dysphagia clinicians in medical settings. however, the body of clinicians maintain the sentiment that these methods fail to sufficiently meet the knowledge base and experience required to clinically or instrumentally manage dysphagic patients. these challenges have not only surfaced in the clinical area of dysphagia, but have also presented in other areas of clinical science with expanding knowledge bases. the end result has been the development of specialty recognition programs in specific areas of clinical practice by the american medical association. in addition, health care professions such as dentistry, pharmacy, physical therapy, occupational therapy, and nursing have also implemented specialty recognition programs that encompass competency training, measurement and methods for recognition. the primary incentive of these programs was not to embellish the concept of specialty practice in an age when general practitioners are becoming the preferred health care model, rather to "... ensure the welfare, safety, comfort, and quality of care of the public consumer" (report on the asha ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, 1994). even though the health care reform activities in the united states (e.g., shift to highly managed medical care) has limited the patient's options in their selection of health care providers, specialty recognition provides a vehicle for all consumers, including the individual patient, payers, and in some cases employers to identify clinical professionals with specialty skills that best meet their health care needs. the american-speech-language-hearing association is no exception to the professional organizations that have witnessed an expanding scope of practice among its members because of the evolving body of information that has resulted as an outgrowth of clinical research, experience ** dr. bonnie martin was an invited guest of saslhaand presented workshops on dysphagia at various venues during 1995 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 bonnie j.w. martin and technological advancements. the legislative council (lc) of asha approved a position statement explaining that the scope of practice in the profession has expanded and involves a "... broad range of services offered within the profession" (lc 6-89). position statements and clinical guidelines were developed for several areas of practice that were also submitted to and adopted by the lc. the clinical areas encompassed in these documents included assessment and management of oral myofunctional disorders (asha, 1991a), learning disability (asha, 1991b), language learning disorders (asha, 1982a), minority language populations (asha, 1983b; 1985), mental retardation (asha, 1982b), cognitive communication impairments (asha, 1988), balance system assessment (asha, 1992a), electrical stimulation for cochlear implant selection and rehabilitation (asha, 1992b), cerumen management (asha, 1992d) and dysphagia (asha, 1992e; asha 1991a; asha 1991b; asha 1982a; asha 1982b; asha 1983b; asha 1988; asha 1992a; asha 1992b; asha 1992d; asha 1992e). continued technical advances obviated the need for additional guidelines in the areas of augmentative and alternative communication (asha, 1991c), neurophysiologic intraoperative monitoring (asha, 19920, tracheoesophageal iistulization procedures (asha, 1992c), and vocal tract visualization and imaging (asha, 1992g; asha 1991c; asha1992f; asha 1992c; asha 1992g). most of the position statements and guidelines describe the range of proficiencies, knowledge bases and competencies required for provision of services by a clinician in the specific area of clinical practice (report of the asha ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, 1994). concurrent with the efforts to detail position statements and practice guidelines in specialty areas of the profession, special interest divisions were approved (lc 35-86) in 1987 and implemented in 1991 in an attempt to provide a structure in which asha colleagues with similar clinical and research interests could interact and exchange information. the development of the special interest divisions were one part of a two part initiative established by the ad hoc committee on specialty recognition report in 1986 (lc 35-86). in 1992 the dysphagia special interest division 13 was formed, and grew to be the largest division in the association in 1995. the division has a steering committee that meets periodically, and the entire division is invited to assemble annually at the national convention of asha. a quarterly newsletter is also published that informs the division members of current clinical and research activities in the area of dysphagia, and offers a forum for professional interchange. the second part of the ad hoc committee on specialty recognition report (lc35-86) included development of a plan to recognize individuals demonstrating a particular expertise in an area(s) of clinical practice. several models were developed and considered by the association. in 1992 at the association's convention in san antonio, texas, the ad hoc committee on specialty certification consulted with the special interest divisions' board of coordinators and with selected sids at their membership meetings. the issue of specialty certification was addressed and the study of specialty certification was endorsed. in the context of the dysphagia special interest division 13 membership meeting, participants expressed verbal support of the specialty certification initiatives, but there continued to be concern regarding the limited educational and clinical opportunities available at the graduate level in university speech pathology programs that would assist clinicians in achieving specialty certification in the area of dysphagia. in an attempt to address the issue of limited formal training opportunities raised by the sid 13 members, a task force of the division was formed whose charge was to devise a suggested graduate core curriculum for accredited speech-language pathology programs in colleges and universities throughout the united states. the format of the curriculum includes a basic graduate level lecture course with practical lab and observations, as well as suggestions to instructors for reference materials and clinical practicum. in addition, an advanced level course structured and recommended for individuals who desire further training in swallowing and research in dysphagia was also included. recommended clinical contact hours for the post-graduate clinical fellowship year were suggested. the recommended core curriculum will be reviewed by the sid 13 membership and forwarded to the educational standards board (esb) of asha. this initiative represents a critical step forward toward the enhancement of the theoretical and working knowledge of entry level dysphagia clinicians. in addition to graduate core curriculum and cfy contact hours, the specialty task force also recognized the need for speech pathologists to be able to demonstrate basic clinical competencies in the work setting prior to treating the often medically and behaviourally complex dysphagic patient. dysphagia management often involves relatively invasive methods that have not been traditionally utilized by clinicians in the field of speech-language pathology. also, treatment recommendations and methods can impact directly on the medical status, nutrition, and safety of the patient. further, the health care industry, including third party payers, will demand improved functional outcomes that can only be provided by highly competent dysphagia clinicians. therefore, the task force endorses that the specific work setting establish basic clinical competencies for dysphagia clinicians that may be very specific to the environment and needs of a particular patient population. at the evelyn trammell voice and swallowing center of saint joseph's hospital of atlanta, a model clinical competency training program has been devised and implemented that encompasses training modules, direct observations, supervised and independent contact hours and continuing education vehicles in the areas of swallowing assessment and treatment. in addition, if clinicians will practice in highly specialized areas of the hospital, such as critical care units, the clinician must meet clinical competencies that relate to the medically complex and unstable patient (martin, martin & cobb, 1993). dysphagic patients, particularly those in the critical care setting often present with multisystem dysfunction that impact upon their communication and swallowing status. the physiologic implications of 'whole body sick' on communication and swallowing functions, however, are often poorly understood and neglected when evaluating and planning dysphagia treatment. speech pathologists are traditionally trained in the neurologic and respiratory systems as they relate to speech, voice and language. however, knowledge of body multisystem influences on speech and swallowing abilities is often incomplete. swallowing and swallowing therapy methods have been shown to produce changes in the respiratory system, and these the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3. 4. provides the highest probability that future changes in the clinical services in a particular area can be accommodated in changes in the competency verification mechanisms applied to the discipline; the model places the burden of responsibility for development and maintenance of the recognition program in the hands of the practicing clinicians (report of the asha ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, 1994). dysphagia evaluation and management: clinical training, clinical competency and speciality recognition changes must be recognized and considered in planning safe and appropriate dysphagia treatments (martin, 1991; martin, corlew, wood, olson, et al., 1993; martin, haynes, mcconnel, o'connor, haring & bouis, 1994; martin, logemann, shaker & dodds, 1993a; martin, logemann, shaker & dodds, 1993b; nishino, yonezawa & honda, 1985; selly, flack, ellis & brooks, 1986; smith, wolkove, colacone & kreisman 1987). further, dysphagia clinicians often evaluate and treat patients in the critical care units who undergo continuous cardiopulmonary monitoring. one purpose of these visual monitoring devices is to allow attending clinicians to modify their treatment plans based on the physiologic responses of the patient during the treatment sessions. this is troubling because the dysphagia clinician typically has not been trained in the basic interpretation of these physiologic visual signals, and leads to intimidation and incompetency in treating the critical care patient. also, the functional status of critical care patients and their ability to tolerate swallowing therapy will vary linearly with their medical status. therefore, the dysphagia clinician should become familiar with the clinical significance of relevant laboratory values, vital signs, pharmacological agents, pulmonary and radiographic tests that are typically reported in the patient's medical record. competency training by dysphagia clinicians in these specialty skill areas has been incorporated into saint joseph's model because of the highly specialized tertiary nature of the facility. the competency training has resulted in elevation of the dysphagia clinicians' clinical insight, skill and confidence when providing care to the medically unstable dysphagic patient. this expertise allows the clinician to begin treatment at an early stage in the patient's recovery, and expedites their return to safe oral intake. demonstration of competency should not only be expected by health care department directors and supervisors, but will be demanded and respected by physicians, patients, family members and other consumers (martin, martin & cobb, 1993). the consumer affairs division of asha also discovered through consumer advocacy group conferences in 1990 and 1992 that consumer groups "strongly supported specialty designations in the j professions as guidance for consumers in selecting providers of services", and appeared to uphold asha's ongoing exploration of the need for a specialty recognition program that goes beyond demonstration of basic clinical competencies as described above (report of the asha ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, 1994). the 1994 report of the ad hoc committee on specialty recognition contains a practitioner-driven model that has been selected as the proposed method for implementing specialty recognition within the asha (report of the asha ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, 1994). the model includes four salient features that highlight maximum participation by practitioners in the field for the development and maintenance of the specialty recognition program, and minimal participation by the central structure of asha: 1. consumer need for recognition of a specialty area can be well defined and justified; 2. practitioners involved in the delivery of services can be responsible for defining the knowledge, skills, and experience requisite to the delivery of services in the specialty area; l the proposed plan as described by the ad hoc committee on specialty recognition maintains a firm commitment to a broad-based practice by the majority of membership, and the concept of nonexclusionary specialty recognition is emphasized throughout the proposal. the plan provides a mechanism by which an individual can be recognized for specialty education experience and expertise, yet assumes that most practitioners will continue to provide broad-based clinical services. the specialty recognition plan is degree independent (report of the asha ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, 1994). because the responsibility for developing the components of the plan has been left to the members of the organization, an additional specialty recognition task force was formed by the dysphagia special interest division 13 in 1995, and a proposed specialty recognition program plan was devised. a draft of the dysphagia proposal will be presented to interested members of asha at the national convention in orlando, florida in december, 1995. the proposed program is highly competency based, and incorporates objective methods for competency measurement. in the plan proposed by the ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, a formal petitioning group submits the final dysphagia specialty certification plan to a clinical specialty board (csb), and a commission on dysphagia would be formed if the group's application has met the specialty recognition standards. the specialty commission on dysphagia would be responsible for maintaining the professional process for accepting, reviewing, maintaining, and renewing applications for recognition by dysphagia clinicians (report of the asha ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, 1994). while the dysphagia specialty certification program is in its infancy proposal stage, it represents a hallmark initiative toward the insurance of exemplary quality dysphagia care by recognized professionals who could potentially serve as clinical competency instructors to novice clinicians in the field. improved quality of care, patient outcomes and cost containment result from reductions in variability of practice and increases in standardization of specialty patient care. a comprehensive graduate core curriculum, clinical competency training and specialty recognition are modalities that will ultimately lead to improvement in the standard of care for dysphagic individuals provided by speech-language pathologists. the expanding scope of practice in the field of speech-language pathology is not unique to the united states. professional associations of clinicians from other countries will likely meet similar challenges with the issues of ensuring appropriate education, clinical competency, quality outcomes and specialty recognition. they will need to face these challenges with opportunity by tailoring methods and vehicles to meet the needs of their swallowing practitioners and health care consumers. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 references american speech-language-hearing association (1982a, november). language learning disorders. asha, pp. 937944. american speech-language-hearing association (1982b, august). serving the communicatively handicapped mentally retarded individual. asha, pp. 547-553. american speech-language-hearing association (1983b, september). social dialects (and implications). asha, pp. 23-27. american speech-language-hearing association (1988, march). the role of speech-language pathologists in the identification, diagnosis, and treatment of individuals with cognitivecommunicative impairments. asha, p. 79. american speech-language-hearing association (1991a, march). the role of the speech-language pathologist in assessment and management of oral myofunctional disorders, asha (suppl. 5), p. 7. american speech-language-hearing association (1991b, march). learning disabilities: issues on definition. asha (suppl. 5), pp. 18-20. american speech-language-hearing association (1991c, march). augmentative and alternative communication. asha (suppl. 5), pp. 9-12. american speech-language-hearing association ad hoc committee on advances in clinical practice (1992a, march). balance system assessment. asha (suppl. 7), pp. 9-12. american speech-language-hearing association ad hoc committee on advances in clinical practice (1992b, march). electrical stimulation for cochlear implant selection and rehabilitation. asha (suppl. 7), pp. 13-16. american speech-language-hearing association ad hoc committee on advances in clinical practice (1992c, march). evaluation and treatment for tracheoesophageal fistulization/ puncture. asha (suppl. 7), pp. 17-21. american speech-language-hearing association ad hoc committee on advances in clinical practice (1992d, march). external auditory canal examination and cerumen management. asha (suppl. 7), pp. 22-24. american speech-language-hearing association ad hoc committee on advances in clinical practice (1992e, march). instrumental diagnostic procedures for swallowing. asha (suppl. 7), pp. 25-33. american speech-language-hearing association ad hoc committee on advances in clinical practice (1992f, march). neurophysiology intraoperative monitoring. asha (suppl. 7), pp. 34-36. bonnie j.w. martin american speech-language-hearing association ad hoc committee on advances in clinical practice (1992g, march). vocal tract visualization and imaging. asha (suppl. 7), pp. 37-40. asha omnibus survey, personal communication from herb bauma, asha, february 27, 1995. martin, b.j. w. (1991). the influence of deglutition on respiration. doctoral dissertation, northwestern university. martin, b.j.w., & corlew, m.m. (1990). the incidence of communication disorders in dysphagic patients. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 55, 28-32. martin, b.j.w., corlew, m.m., wood, h., olson, d., et al. (1993). the association of swallowing dysfunction and aspiration pneumonia. dysphagia, 9 (1). martin, b.j.w., logemann, j.a., shaker, r., & dodds, w.j. (1993a). normal laryngeal valving maneuvres during three breath hold maneuvres: a pilot investigation. dysphagia, 8, pp. 11-20. martin, b.j.w., haynes, r., mcconnel, f.m.s., o'connor, α., haring, k., bouis, h. (1994). breathing and swallowing interrelationships. dysphagia research society meeting, mclean, virginia, october 14, 1994. martin, b.j.w., logemann, j.a., shaker, r., & dodds, w.j. (1993b). the coordination between respiration and swallow: respiratory phase relationships and temporal integration. journal of applied physiology, 76 (2), pp. 714-123. martin, b.j.w., martin, e., cobb, r. (1993). critical care competencies for dysphagia clinicians. short course presented at the annual meeting of the american speechlanguage-hearing association, anaheim, california. nishino, r., yonezawa, r., & honda, y. (1985). effects of swallowing on the pattern of continuous respiration in human adults. american review of respiratory disease, 132, pp. 12191222. report of the asha ad hoc committee on specialty recognition, august, 1994. selly, w., flack, f„ ellis, r. & brooks, w. (1986). respiratory patterns associated with swallowing: parti. the normal adult pattern and changes with age. age and ageing, 18, pp. 168172. simmons, k. (1986). dysphagia management means diagnosis, exercise, re-education. journal of the american medical association, 255, 3209-3210, 3212. smith, j., wolkove, n., colacone, α., & kreisman (1987). coordination of eating, drinking and breathing in adults, chest, 96, pp. 578-582. ο x m play a n d s c h o o l r o o m i i ο x m shop 6l the rosebank mews 173 oxford road rosebank jhb. i phone 788-1304 fax: 880-1341 tests, programmes, books, teaching aids, journals and resources for child development speech & language learning disabilities special needs adult rehabilitation social activities po box 52137 saxonwold 2132 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 harsha kathard, mershen pillay, michael samuel and vijay reddy genesis of self-identity as disother: life histories of people who stutter harsha kathard*, mershem pillayi, michael samuel 0 amd vijay reddyd •division of communication sciences and disorders, university of cape town, south africa $sheik khalifa hospital, abu dhabi, united arab emirates ° school of educational studies, university of kwazulu natal, south africa nhuman sciences research council, pretoria, south africa abstract this paper explores the processes shaping self-identity formation as disother and the actions of participants who stutter. it illuminates the experiences of adults who stutter using a biographical, narrative, life history methodology. the participants were seven south african adults of diverse racial, social and economic backgrounds from kwazulu natal, south africa. five males and two females were invited to participate via purposive and convenience sampling processes. their stories of living with stuttering in their life worlds over time were constructed via biographical interviews using personal, social and temporal lenses typical of life history methodology. the interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed. the data were analysed at two levels using a combination of strategies. the first level entailed a narrative analysis that was represented as research stories for each participant. the cross-case and thematic analysis of research stories constituted the second level analysis of narratives. the findings explain the complex and interrelated personal and social processes over time which contribute to the genesis of self-identity formation as disother. social inscriptions of difference occurred in immediate home, school and work contexts over time via multiple processes such as labelling, norming, judging and'teasing. personal processes included discoveries of difference via critical events, repeated reinforcement of difference, self-judgement and temporal burdening. furthermore, the actions participants took in negotiating stuttering were examined. the implications of the findings and limitations of the study are presented. key words: self-identity formation, disother, people who stutter, life history. introduction this paper illuminates the processes shaping self-identity formation as disother of participants who stutter and their actions in relation to their self-identity, using a narrative life history methodology. the term "disother", constructed by pillay (2003), was appropriated in this paper to refer to an individual's understanding of himself as problematically different because he/she stuttered. the term was derived from two sources. firstly, the term "other" refers to instances when an individual manufactures himself as problematically different in a particular context (boehmer, 1995). it carries a meaning of an individual feeling he/she has intrinsically less value. secondly, the prefix "dis" refers to instances where the individual feels different, threatened, destabilised and disempowered on the basis of his disorder, in this instance, the disorder being the stutter. the rationale for this study emerged from two central concerns. firstly, there has been limited research on the personal and social dimensions of the stuttering experience. quesal (1989) challenged the professions' researchers for a lack of engagement with the core psychosocial and personal concerns of people who stutter (pws) in an article aptly titled "have we forgotten the stutterer?" he argued cogently that as a consequence of difficulties in quantifying psychosocial factors these issues were being interpreted as a lack of reality, that is, if we can't count or measure it, it does not exist. over time, the importance of personal experience research has been recognised and research on varied dimensions has gathered gradual momentum (corcoran & stewart, 1998; crichton-smith, 2002; die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 kathard, 2003; petrunik & shearing, 1983; sue-o'brien, 1993). research on dimensions of self and stuttering viz. on selfconception and self-esteem has been a source of professional research interest over time (green, 1997; green, 1999; kalinowski, lerman & watt, 1997; van riper, 1982; yovetich, leschied & flicht 2000; zelen, sheehan & bugenthal, 1954). while these studies have examined varied dimensions of selfconcept, they have not researched the processes shaping selfidentity formation over time. furthermore, they differ methodologically from this study because they relied on quantitative methodologies to understand varied relationships between self-conception and severity (green, 1999) and assessments of self-esteem (yovetich, leschied & flicht, 2000). green (1999) however, suggested that complex relationships between environmental, personal and behavioural factors influencing the self-conception of children could be enhanced by drawing on experience histories of pws. this study expands the methodological toolbox by introducing narrative biographical methodology in exploring self-identity formations over time. secondly, the profession of speech-language pathology has the responsibility of intervening with pws and requires a relevant personal experience knowledge base to inform intervention. adults who stutter, like all people with communication disorders, present a challenge to the clinician'because they bring a lifetime of experiences into the clinical relationship. interaction with the client's life experience is an important prerequisite for intervention (bloodstein, 1995; van riper, 1982). the limited research thus far could be attributed to the methodological tradition of the profession that has relied r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) genesis of self-identity as disother: life histories of people who stutter 5 primarily on positivist, quantitative and experimental designs (mowrer, 1998; perkins, 1997; pillay, 2003). silverman (2001, p. 4) provides a succinct critique of how the research outcomes within a positivist frame have (not) served clinical practice: the fact remains that people seeking help with stuttering problems are just that people. and the information that therapists have from those conducting research and writing books i.e. the means, standard deviations, etc. simply don't address that fact very well. the method of science with all its assumptions about reality from a human perspective including the need for objectivity of the so-called observer, linearity of experience and the uses of inferential and descriptive statistical analyses to interpret observations simply can not, at this point in space-time, generate information completely useful to modify behaviours of multitasking, complexly functioning human beings. personal, more than impersonal, knowledge is required to inaugurate, modify, stabilise and maintain behaviour change. narrative methodologies are robust ways of researching experience (plummer, 2001) because stories best facilitate researchers' understanding of experiences (clandinin & connelly, 2000). sacks (1995, p. 25) explained the value of the self-story in understanding how people live with an impairment/ disorder and the actions they take, "ask not what disease the person has, but rather what person has the disease." the answer to the first question is a monological chart (about disorder/ impairment). the answer to the second question will always be a story. unlike disease, no two people will have the same story. he proposed that the study of identity and disease/disorder could not be separated in personal experience research. researchers therefore should attend to the important "who am i?" question as a basis for understanding how the person makes sense of his/her worlds and how he/she acts. life history methodology1 is useful in exploring selfidentification formations because it admits the personal, temporal and social dimensions of experience. it attends to the critical temporal dimension 'of self-identity formations by considering the changing life cycle of the individual to be taken as'a single unit of study (hatch & wisniewski, 1995). it foregrounds the connection! of the experience to social circumstances and positions the participant as an active storyteller, thereby allowing for an explanation of social action from a personal perspective. : there are many theoretical debates about the notion of self-identity which are beyond the scope of this paper. therefore, the theoretical frame adopted in this study is presented here. self-identities only become identities when people internalise them (mishler, 1999). therefore, the concept of self-identity must be differentiated from roles and role-sets. while people may have many roles, identities are sources of meaning that actors construct for themselves through processes of individuation. although identities may coincide with roles, identities are stronger sources of meaning (castells, 1997). self-identities are always in a state of process, of "becoming" (mishler, 1999). the term "formation" was chosen to reinforce the process orientation of the self, past, present and future. it shifts away from the stage model of development which have traditionally enforced a rigid, orderly universal ά fuller explanation of the application of life history methodology can be found in kathard 2003. and progressive understanding of self-identity formations. identities have the capacity to be stable or flexible over time (valsiner, 2002). the process of narrative self-identity formation occurs by drawing together the overlapping cognitive, emotional, temporal, · relational, macro-structural, cultural, institutional, and moral dimensions, i.e. the personal and social influences (somers, 1994). social psychology theorists (howard, 2000) have emphasised issues of ethnicity, gender, class, age, disability, race, and geography in shaping self-identity formation. societies are multiple, fluid and changing and it is within this relational social matrix that self-identities form. within the relational matrix, the impairment/bodily dimensions (in this instance stuttering) are socially interpreted. in traditional medical science, disorders have been studied without the self. the notion of self-as-embodied however, humanises the body and dissolves the traditional boundaries between body/impairment, self and society. when impairment or disorder exist, people with disabilities (pwd) are constructed as an epistemological "other" (perry, 1996) at the social interface. goffman's (1963) theory on spoiled identity has been used to understand experiences of disability or "otherness". a person who is stigmatised is a person whose social identity or membership to some social category, calls into question his or her full humanity the person is devalued, spoiled or flawed in the eyes of others. "othering" (fine, 1998) does not occur only on the basis of impairments, but also on dimensions of race, gender, sexuality, class or education. however, in research with disability there has been a tendency for the emergence of neat, clean and categorical understandings of identity foregrounding disability. such understandings mask the complexity of identity formations in pwd. in the light of such social complexity the moral impulses of self-identity formations must be understood against the backdrop of societal norms and values. frank (2002) emphasised the importance of listening to the moral impulse of the self-story because self-identity formations are reflective of the social-moral debate i.e. who am i? and what it means to be good or bad in a society. it would also seem important to explore how participants negotiate stuttering in relation to their selfidentity formations. "i act because i am" suggests that the actions of individuals should be linked with their identity formations. it was also of value to understand why people act as they do and to guard against limiting understanding to universalist, essentialist notions (somers, 1994), for example expecting that all pws will act the same way. this stance makes provision for participants with impairment to be understood as agentic (frank, 2002), in contrast to passive, pathology-based stereotypic views. method aims the aims in this paper have been extrapolated from a larger study. in the study, the self-identity trajectories as able/potential and self-identity as disother emerged i.e. participants understood themselves as able and as disother. this paper is limited the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 harsha kathard, mershen pillay, michael samuel and vijay reddy to the exploration of processes of self-identity formation as disother and thus the aims were: 1. to explore the processes over time shaping self-identity formation as disother of participants who stutter. 2. to illuminate the actions of participants in the context of their self-identity formations as disother. participants participants in the main study were required to be adults (over 18 years of age) who had stuttered since early childhood, as the intention of the study was to explore the experiences of stuttering over their life courses. they had to be willing to share their stories in a voluntary capacity and indicate their commitment to participate in a process requiring prolonged engagement. furthermore, they were required to have information rich stories (plummer, 2001), and english as a functional language of communication. the researcher was english-speaking and because the interview process required close and interpretive communication, it was felt that a shared language between participants was essential. participation was invited through a combination of purposive and convenience sampling procedures. participants were recruited via local hospitals, private practices, the university, and the local stuttering self-help group. of the 10 people sampled, 7 met the criteria for selection. the profiles of participants are summarised in table 1. the information reflects the status of the participants at the time of the study. the severity of stuttering was determined by participants' self-rating of stuttering severity at the time of the interview. data production the data was produced via interviews with each participant. at the outset,the nature of the research process, voluntary participation and withdrawal, communication (for example, conversational repair strategies, formality of the conversation) and confidentiality were discussed. the management of power imbalances inherent in the research context was negotiated with participants with the intention of developing a respectful research relationship (measor & sykes, 1992). the process commenced after participants had a full understanding of the research process and had consented in writing. a semistructured, open-ended life history interview schedule was constructed to support the interview. in narrative research, the researcher features as an instrument (hany, 1996) and it was necessary for the researcher to be reflexive and critical (peshkin, 2001) about how she was influencing the data production process. this process was monitored by peer critique and reflective memo-writing (charmaz, 1995). in particular, the researcher had to be cognisant of the need to be non-judgemental and to appreciate the uncertain nature of the interview process. a panasonic mini-cassette tape-recorder (rq-l30) was used to record all interviews. the interview process unfolded differently with each participant. in general, the initial session was used to establish rapport and to place the interview within the context of the study. in subsequent sessions, participants were asked to relate their stories beginning with their early experiences of stuttering and how they acted. a strict temporal line was not pursued because participants moved back and forth through their experiences. the process was deliberately open to allow participants to select the issues and events they felt were most important. the researcher probed these aspects further using matrix-type probing techniques to explore critical interfaces between the personal, temporal and social dimensions of experience. it was necessary to constantly monitor the potential for power imbalances in the interview and to create a discursive space in which participants felt comfortable in sharing their stories. the interview process was considered to be an interpretive conversation (josselson, 1995; fine, 1998) in which the researcher and participants were engaged in a process of joint meaning-making. this conversation required an empathetic listening and "sensing" and "connecting" (kathard, 2003) with participants. each interview lasted for approximately two hours and the total interview time for participants ranged from 6 hours to 10 hours. participants were each interviewed an average of three times. table 1: biographical profiles of participants participant gender age residential area race severity of stuttering therapy occupation / education | gareth male 65 durban white mild yes retired architect; , university hennie male 29 pinetown white moderate to severe yes accountant; university siyanda male 32 inanda black moderate no director: arts association; high school: standard nine thabo male 19 ashdown black severe yes university student kumari female 36 shallcross indian mild no accountant; university ' sagren male 32 pinetown indian moderate yes manager; university nontokozo female 20 umlazi black severe yes university student die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ^ genesis of self-identity as disother: life histories of people who stutter 7 data analysis each interview was transcribed verbatim from audio-tape recordings for each participant. the accuracy of the transcriptions was checked by the researcher and a research assistant to ensure the actual words spoken were correctly transcribed especially when speech intelligibility was influenced by a stutter. the written transcripts constituted the data and were subjected to analysis. given that the written transcription can never be a faithful copy of the interview (kvale, 1996), the researcher interacted with the written transcriptions as well as the audio recordings and memos. the data analysis was conducted at two levels. the first level entailed a representational narrative analysis (freeman, 1996; polkinghome, 1996). in narrative analysis, the raw data are configured by means of a plot into a story, thereby moving from elements to stories to explain a particular end. the product reflects a temporal ordering in which each part is given meaning via its reciprocal relationship with other parts, before and after, and to the whole. the plot facilitates the composition of events into a story (polkinghome, 1995) by: • clarifying the meaning events have as contributors to the story; • providing criteria for the selection of events/issues to be included; • delimiting the temporal range which marks the beginning and end of the story; • providing temporal ordering and unfolding of events leading to a conclusion. this level of analysis was represented as a research story for each participant. the second level of analysis involved a mixed strategy (reddy, 2000) of grounding the analysis within the individual case as well as constant comparisons across cases. an iterative strategy of analysis was used (charmaz, 1995). the researcher moved backwards and forwards between the interview data, i research stories and emerging categories on a continuous basis to generate constructs and themes. two processes of validation were used in the study viz. substantive and ethical validation. substantive validation was concerned with issues of trustworthiness and goodness of interpretive research (angen, 2000) and required consideration of issues like suitability of methodological choices, credibility, dependability and authenticity trail (creswell & miller, 2000; lincoln & guba, 1985). in contrast, ethical validation was concerned with the moral issues the researcher must entertain. this study utilized lincoln and guba's (1985) guidelines to enhance substantive and ethical validity. • member checking was used during the interview process to confirm technical details, obtain clarity on particular issues and to verify the researcher's interpretation of the story. participants also had access to the transcripts and were provided with two versions of their research stories. they were invited to comment on the trustworthiness of the representation of their stories and their comments were taken into consideration in further revision of their research stories. • prolonged engagement (plummer, 2001) ensured that the data was collected in a rigorous manner. this engagement included a few indepth interviews which totalled several hours as well as ongoing consultation with participants over time, extending over a year. • peer debriefing and critique was done by two research colleagues with speech-language pathology backgrounds, and one researcher with life history research experience. they were familiar with the study and provided guidance throughout the research process to ensure that it was rigorous, fair and thorough. • thick descriptions of empirical data were provided and represented as research stories. thick descriptions capture detail, density and depth to create a sense of verisimilitude so that the statements and stories can bring the experience to life. the reader then feels as if he/she could experience the events being described (charmaz, 1995; plummer, 2001). • procedural dependability was ensured via an audit trail analogous to a fiscal audit. all aspects of the research process were monitored by two peers (life history researchers) external to the study to confirm that the research process was systematic and rigorous. ethical validation addressed potential power imbalances during the research process. the fairness criterion (lincoln & guba, 1985) attempted to ensure that the interview process was fair, that voices were not demeaned, silenced and that the potential power held by the researcher or participant was restrained. the research study received ethical clearance from university authorities and informed consent was obtained from participants after clarifying issues of confidentiality, voluntary participation, withdrawal, risks and benefits. member checking, open communication and self-critique characterized the interaction to enhance ethical validity. a counseling support system, external to the research process, was available if participants felt the need for support. as every research process has potential to touch the participant in some way, there was a vigilant effort to reduce harm (frank, 2001). results and discussion the presentation is arranged in relation to the aims of the study. the data are represented as excerpts from the biographical research stories of participants. the emerging issues included: ·. discovering difference and the emergent self-identity as disother ° processes of strengthening, reinforcing and sedimenting self-identity formation as disother • negotiating self-identity as disother (action) discovering difference and emergent self-identity as disother all participants in this study began to stutter during the preschool years with the onset of stuttering occurring before 6 years of age and with differing degrees of severity. the primary intention was to examine the circumstances through which participants began to discover themselves as different. critical incidents and contexts for discovering difference i remember the first day i discovered the stutter. this happened when i was in grade two, about seven years old. i was the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 harsha kathard, mershen pillay, michael samuel and vijay reddy table 2: incidents and events leading to the initial discovery of self as different participants critical events and primary contexts age: discovering difference age: onset of stuttering siyanda school: teacher punished him for not speaking on cue 8 3 kumari home: child abused by alcoholic father, was silent and then stuttered. she understood stuttering as a sign of fear and disempowerment 4 4 gareth home: parents' extreme concern at a young age about speech fluency 4 3 sagren home: early concern at home about poor speech development 3 3 thabo school: mocked by class mates when unable to read 7 4 nonthokozo school: critical incidents of feeling different within a new school environment and referral for treatment 13 5 hennie school: high school years: adolescent identity questioning 12 5 reading a book in front of the children in class. i was very shy and nervous and as a result i started stuttering suddenly. the children started to laugh. at that moment i wished the ground would just open so i could get under it and die. i was so very hurt that i nearly cried too. at first i didn 't even know what the stutter was. i didn't even know why they were laughing. that was the beginning of my nightmare. before that day i knew nothing of the stutter. no one at home said anything about it. (thabo) at home, the way we spoke, with our stutter, wasn't a problem. outside home it was a different story. i was about eight years old. we went to school in umlazi, an african township, in the 1970s and the rules were strict. one of the english teachers asks a question. if you don't know the answer then you stand up. he asks me a question. i am in the process of a stutter. i am trembling and i can't get it out. he hits me. that is my first memory when i was punished for not answering a question on time because of the stutter. i broke the rules. (siyanda) i started to stutter when i was young, around three or four years old. i was a very sensitive little chap and remember my parents worrying about my speech. i knew something was wrong. i suspect it was bad from early on. they said the usual fatherly and motherly thing like "slow down, take your time". i was aware of the stuttering at that stage but i don't think i was too troubled by it... at the time, on some occasions there were these verbal collisions at home. my parents, both of staid english backgrounds, were a difficult match. so, a degree of collision took place.! when these mws were on, for me, a sensitive little boy, it was like the end of the world. i don't know how it was for my older brother. those collisions made more of an impact on me then, than did my faulting speech. (gareth) i started talking very late, when i was four years old. girls normally speak early but i didn't. there was a reason for this which only i know. my family wondered what was going wrong. they thought i might have a problem with my ears or tongue or brain. they were wrong. it was fear, a gruesome, monstrous, all-consuming fear that silenced me. i never spoke for four years. when i did, it came out ssssstuttering. fear caused my stuttering. the fear was like venom, which spread quickly to every part of me, every crevice of my mind, body and spirit. a deep-seated ugly, emotional fear. i tried hard to shrug off but it still lurked in me, unrelenting. what caused this fear? the man of the house, my father. (kumari) i always had a small stutter but the problem started in high school in 1982. my parents sent me to a convent boarding school. this was a very big change for me. i didn't want to go there. everything was so strange. the school was run by white german nuns. the nuns were very strict. we follow rules. they spoke english. we were black, zulu speaking girls but only allowed to speak english. you couldn't go off the school premises. i was free before. wake up early. walk in a straight line. behave like girls. sleep early. i was sad and out-of place and the stutter became very severe. from a hiccup i went to having big, long blocks. (nontokozo). the narratives revealed that the contexts for discovering difference were their homes and schools, their immediate living contexts. parents, teachers and peers drew attention to stuttering as being different to normal and as a disorder by reacting to it in a negative way. the incidents ranged from casual correcting to mocking and had variable impacts on participants who then began to create initial understandings of themselves | as different. although these critical incidents were varied in nature across participants, they remained robust and were imprinted in participants' memories. as a consequence, they had a pervasive influence on participants' discoveries of themselves as different and began to shape their self-identity as disother. the social processes and interpretations of stuttering were not consistent between and within stories. for example, siyanda's and thabo's experiences of stuttering at home were different to that of school. in kumari's story the trend was reversed. these experiences highlighted that stuttering was not interpreted and acted on uniformly across contexts and that there were multiple interpretations of stuttering within a single life experience. however, it was also apparent that despite the varying and competing interpretations'of stuttering, participants eventually gained an understanding of themselves as different by their adolescent years. the negative interpretations were die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) genesis of self-identity as disother: life histories of people who stutter 9 conveyed to them through a variety of social responses which suggested that it was wrong to stutter that they had broken rules that they were different. there was variation in the ages at which participants discovered themselves as different (table 2). while five participants in this study began to understand themselves as different at an early age, 3 to 8 years of age during the primary school years, two (hennie and nontokozo) were aware of their speech being different but did not attach strong meaning to themselves as different until adolescence. this trend suggested uneveness in their biographical circumstances and that varied contexts and processes contributed to initial shaping of their self-identity as disother. importantly, the onset of their stuttering i.e. the presence of impairment and the development of a self-identity as disother did not coincide. stuttering in itself was therefore not sufficient to begin to construct themselves as different. the impairment/disorder must receive a (negative) social interpretation to shape one's self-identity as disother. fertile ground for discovering difference: complex realities and vulnerable selves the stories also revealed that for some participants (gareth, sagren, kumari, nontokozo) the circumstances for discovering difference were complex. within these realities, participants were potentially vulnerable and "powerless" to varying degrees. kumari in particular, attributed her silence and subsequent stutter directly to her sense of vulnerability and fearfulness as a child in an abusive environment. she explained stuttering as a symptomatic manifestation of fear of her abusive father. similarly, gareth had a heightened sense of being vulnerable and "at-risk". a similar set of circumstances emerged in sagren's story. siyanda, thabo and nonthokozo were relatively powerless in difficult school contexts. biographical suicide (samuel, 1998) describes nontokozo's context of feeling vulnerable. there was a devaluation and obliteration of her history as she was ̂ expected to take on the values of a powerful and dominant new system alien to her. in this context she felt vulnerable and "less than". these interplay of circumstances contributed to an exacerbation of stuttering through which she formed her selfidentity as disother. | the participants' "fragility" amid these difficult home and school environments created fertile ground to develop a selfidentity as disother. it is possible that another child, such as their siblings, with a different personal makeup, may not have been influenced in a similar way in the same situation. it is equally possible that the same child in another context might not have been as vulnerable. hence, the combinations of their vulnerable "at risk" selves embedded in difficult social contexts contributed to their early understandings of being different. reinforcing, strengthening and sedimenting self-identity at school... the threesome, the principal, the nurse and the school inspector. society watchdogs! we knew what they were looking for, all the misfits, all the problems. maybe they would forget about me. then they start: come to the front when we call your name and problem: mcdonald and hastings headlice, smith can't see well, lovemore cripple, blake stutterer. i just want to disappear into the ground. is this all they know about me? hey, remember i came second. they fill out the forms. i remain silent. powerless. they make it unbearable. everyone knows i am a stutterer but this is a painful public display. when you are young the last thing you want anyone to say is that you are different or you have a problem. nothing came of that incident but i did eventually get to therapy. she tried to help but i hated it because it intruded on all the joys... my sport and all that stuff... i was just fed up with it... just relax, relax... it didn't do me any good. it just emphasised my difference and that i didn 't speak well. (gareth) at high school i was still being teased. i could never say my name or answer a question without being stuck. there was the one time when we were all assembled at the beginning of the year so they could place us in new classes. the teacher calls the names of each child. she calls my name. silence. i have a block at the end the numbers don't tally and the principal wants to know where the problem is. the teacher whispers to him that i stutter. he announces loudly that she must ask me questions everyday so i get used to speaking. i am sure he was trying to help but i felt really embarrassed and it made the situation worse forme. (sagren) the teachers at school noticed my speech. everyone knew the problem. they didn't say anything. they left me to finish what i was saying. it got bad so they arranged a speech therapist for me. i went to therapy at the convent attached to the school. i was happy to go. she taught me to prolong the first word to make my speech fluent. the girls in class would laugh at me so i stopped doing it. they thought this new speech was funny. i stopped going to therapy after a few months. (nonthokozo) workplace... they are hard-nosed business executives. they are the allknowing, economically-driven, powerful, white and rich men. i am going to help get them richer. if you area rep, people don't specifically buy your product but they buy you. it's all about the packaging and presentation. how do i then sell my product. my product is me. i have to sell me in this business i want to own. i'm not really marketable because of the stutter and that's the problem. they will never take someone who stutters seriously. as soon as i stutter they will think i don't know my stuff (hennie) media... the interviewer wasn't knowledgeable and she didn't listen. sagren and i answered the questions. she interpreted the answers and produced a ghastly newspaper article. it conveyed the picture that we were sort of imbecilic and groped for words and had veins sticking out of our necks. of course our veins stick up! but she just conveyed us as poor, poor people with a problem. i read it and didn't like it. although it was redone it still conveyed us as a breed of poor fellows. it was her story and she heard very little of ours. since then i decided i won't talk to the media. (gareth) ongoing social inscriptions of difference: school, media and professional: authorising difference the contexts in which participants continued to strengthen their self-identities as disother over time included the home, school and work contexts. these repeated incidents provided the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 harsha kathard, mershen pillay, michael samuel and vijay reddy impetus for strengthening their otherness. the media was implicated because it publicly portrayed negative images of pws and stuttering. the school as context was significant because participants spent, a minimum of twelve years at school. here their self-identity as disother was reinforced by negative experiences which included: identifying and authorising one as different; judging, evaluating, punishing and teasing within the school community; negative evaluations by teachers and "low marks" for oral work; referrals for treatment by school authorities. the school as community, as an ann of society, repeatedly inscribed stuttering as a problem over time. within a foucauldian (1977) interpretation, the school, among its other functions, also performed the task of surveillance via processes of scanning and detecting it identified problems like stuttering. in cataloguing difference, it drew attention to a "pathology/abnormality" as part of a broader social function. as a symbol of authority in society, the school had named and authorised the problem. while it may be argued that these processes served in the interests of helping children, the stories also revealed how they simultaneously foregrounded problems and difference by formalising and authorising difference thereby generating heightened awareness of norm deviations. in this regard, schools transmitted and sustained a dominant social discourse of stuttering as a problem. in nontokozo, gareth and hennie's stories, schools collaborated with health professionals e.g. nurses, speech therapists to treat stuttering. whilst professional intervention was a means of helping the pupil, it also served to cast stuttering formally into the realm of a disorder and reinforced disotherness through various professional processes. thabo aptly stated, "it is where they will ask: what is wrong with you?" participants had a problem (stuttering) for which there was an official and formal mechanism for treatment. professional interventions are social processes (pillay, 2003), knowledge of which filtered into schools, homes and communities and had potential to reinforce disotherness. these processes occurred repeatedly over time in contexts of daily living. prominent in all their stories were their negative experiences at school. critical transitions: colonising oneself experiencing the moment of stuttering. the funniest thing is you will never know until it's upon you. then you know. oh! my god. there's a block. i didn't know the block was coming. bump and you fall and the block happens. it's a devil of a job to get going again. (gareth) then it happens. out of the blue it takes me by surprise because you don't know exactly when it will pop... you 're not in control of your mouth and that is really annoying me. (hennie) i try to control the stutter but i can't. i try to control the stutter but i can't. takes so long... i ///////////// erer kn... knerer ererererererer erer that i... since you don't understand what i said. i can see by the questioning confused look on you face that you don't understand. here i go again. i can't look at your face. it is so embarrassing. yes. i know what i want to say. i break the contact. you wait. try to guess in your mind. you don't know what i want to say. i continue the struggle since i started it. more than a minute has passed. i stop. try again. finally, it's out. this is only part of what happens. the outside. nothing comes out or too much all at once but you still don't know what i am saying. what goes on inside me is worse. very very bad. the block disability because it takes so long for a word to come out. it happens again and again. (thabo) participants' experiences of the moment of stuttering varied over time. their experiences could be summarised as uncertainty and loss of control, extended and repeated moments of struggle and a feeling that communication was in jeopardy. a constellation of negative emotions accompanied the event. similar reports of suffering have been documented by corcoran and stewart (1998) and sue-o'brien (1993). as this bodily/ impairment experience of stuttering unfolded in a judgemental social world, the experience of "self-out-of-control" emerged and formed the personal layer of experience that contributed to the shaping of their self-identity as disother. self-judging stuttering is standing between me and my dream of becoming a top-class business consultant. i am at the stage where i am really, really fed up. i am at a stage, where i say, if my speech doesn't improve dramatically, then my life is a misery. it comes from the pressure i place on myself when i fail. i just think gee! i sound dof. really dof. i don't even like the sound of my voice when i am fluent. (hennie) when i reached my final year at school, the speech flow was better, but the rest of me, inside, was still an empty hollow. i was detached, isolated and lonely. i had friends, but no best friends because rangini went to another school. my self-esteem was low. rockbottom. i was fashioned out of fear. iam nothing. (kumari) the humiliation is what you suffer because you look stupid. i feel a halfwit. then, you have to pick up the pieces. the whole thing is just so embarrassing. i am not so sure always what people think but i don't want to embarrass them either. (gareth) while social processes were constantly operational, they became influential as participants engaged in personal interpretations of external values and judgements. the personal processes shaping self-identity as disother were therefore critical in the self-identity formation process. there was progression from discovering oneself as different because one stutters to judging oneself as problematically different. the critical transition from "i have a stutter" to "i am a stutterer" and "i am less than" unfolded variably for participants and via different contextual realities reinforcing unevenness between participant experiences. for few participants (thabo and kumari) initial insults and incidents had instantaneous effect and resulted in immediate self-judging and a critical transition to understanding oneself as disother. for most participants this process was more gradual and reinforced by repeated incidents over time. eventually all participants "colonised" themselves, as the dominant and (silent) discourses in school and at home were gradually appropriated into their value systems. hence, all participants discriminated and devalued themselves by borrowing and internalising the oppressive social discourse. temporal burdening the genesis of self-identity as disother occurred over time for all participants. although single incidents remained impordie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) genesis of self-identity as disother: life histories of people who stutter 11 tant in shaping the self-identity formation, temporal burdening was crucial in reinforcing and sedimenting self-identity as disother. kumari's feeling of being a vulnerable child in the presence of an adult male is a good example of the cumulative nature of self-identity formation "i am a little girl in the presence of my boss, a man. i carry the past with me". similarly, thabo's feeling of discomfort in the university classroom occurred as a consequence of an aggregation of past experiences. sagren's story also illuminated the cumulative nature of self-construction as he suggested "take away my memories and much of this problem will be resolved." selfidentity as disother, therefore could be understood as a temporal memory of a burdened self, accumulating suffering over time. multiplicities and intersections of self-identity formations i was always uncomfortable in male company... with male teachers, bosses, my father and father-in-law. i stutter the most in these situations. i was most uncomfortable with white males even if i didn't stutter. (kumari) primary school in umlazi was harsh and we were caught in a vicious cycle with no words. you didn't do your work. ten strokes. come to school late. twelve strokes. the punishment was too heavy. it was not just strict. it was unfair. the situation got bad. we were isolated, reserved and we were punished often. the trouble was not only because of the stutter. at home, we had a broken family. we go home and we see our parents fighting. my father used to beat my mother and we watch. helpless. speechless. this happened for about five years from standard one and standard five. all this stuff put together had a negative impact. we are very silent and in pain. eventually, we just drop out of school in standard five. we were about twelve years old. (siyanda) i entered university and everything was strange. i war just too scared to talk with my stutter and because they were white lecturers. i had never met white people before. i only saw them on/tv. (thabo) j x the participants' formation of self-identity as disother was not based neatly on theirj experience of stuttering but had also been shaped by their multiple positions in society. for example, issues of race, gender and age were indelibly interwoven suggesting that self-identity formations occurred at multiple intersections. the experience of stuttering therefore couldn't be understood solely,on the basis of impairment. kumari's self-identity as other included the collective influences of gender, age, race and stuttering. thabo raised the issues of race and his sense of being other as a black person reared within an apartheid context in south africa. siyanda's early experiences of stuttering began with the classroom and playground incidents. however, he let life slip in and proceeded to share other life-changing experiences which combined to shape his experience of stuttering. he placed these "extraneous experiences" at the centre of his story of stuttering in childhood. his self-identity occurred at multiple intersections as a vulnerable child at home in his "broken family", as poor, as a school dropout and as one who stutters. his "otherness" therefore was not constructed solely on the basis of stuttering. his self-identity as disother was embedded within this complex reality and received meaning within it. negotiating self-identity as disother we spoke only when answering questions and then we just sit down. we were shy guys. (siyanda) i was so hurt. i decided not to speak in class so i don't reveal my weakness. i didn't say a word for two years in class. (thabo) i get so angry when they tease. i have to show them who i am. boomboomboom. i fight. that is only response we have to show that we are irritated. angry! when we are so angry, we can't cough it up. in the classroom we are silent. (siyanda) my stuttering became severe. i war just afraid. stuck stuck. afraid. the only good thing was my best friend rangini. we were well-suited to each other because we were quiet and we silently blended into the background, remaining unnoticed. i was quiet especially in the class because i didn't want them to notice there war something wrong with me. (kumari) on occasion i even try to speak louder than usual, almost shouting to keep fluent, but i feel quite stupid. i would do anything to be a bit fluent but these attempts always failed me. (gareth) pass as normal there are two broad ways in which participants negotiated their self-identities as disother stuttering. firstly, they chose strategies to "normalise" as a primary strategy by remaining silent, concealing the stutter by using a range of techniques and "blending in" to achieve compliance. secondly, they were angered and "fight back". all strategies were contextdependent and fluid. thabo and siyanda chose to be silent in the classroom so that their problems would not be discovered. thabo remained silent and compliant for two years to feign normality and this strategy served a self-protective function. on the playground, repeated teasing, a severe stutter and sense of disempowerment reinforced his silence and consequently threatened his potential social relationships. he felt lonely, isolated, rejected and disconnected which reinforced his selfidentity as disother. sagren, kumari, thabo and siyanda followed similar patterns in the classroom to different degrees. they used strategies of blending in and compliance to reduce attention to their stuttering. for example, they were well-behaved in class and followed rules for games without protesting. when they spoke, they also chose to conceal their stutter using a variety of self-taught techniques or formally-learned strategies. these fluency-inducing strategies were used with varying degrees of success throughout their life paths. gareth suggested that he had difficulty using slow and controlled speech which did not suit his personality but continued to use it because any bit of fluency was welcome. sagren and thabo also used a combination of spontaneous and self-learned concealment strategies to cope with difficult moments. however these strategies frequently failed to conceal the stutter which increased their sense of disempowerment and reinforced their disothemess. the strategies of silence and concealing, blending and compliance can collectively be explained as strategies to "pass as normal" (goffman, 1963; petrunik & shearing, 1983). having realised their disothemess and the disruptive effects of stuttering, participants attempted to become "normal" as a the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 harsha kathard, mershen pillay, michael samuel and vijay reddy means of creating order at the communicative interface. silence was often the first and preferred strategy as it served to reduce the disruption and also became a self-protective function preventing one from being discovered as different or abnormal. other strategies entailed concealing the stutter and attempting to establish fluent speech. concealing is linked to "ordering" as in situations of dis-order, of disruptive communication, participants conceal stuttering to establish interactional order (petrunik & shearing, 1983). one way of achieving this order was to feign fluency or by using any strategy that created order and hence pass as normal. disavowal that's one of the strange things, both my mother and father shouldn't like to talk about these topics... a sticky topic to talk about. the teachers did the same thing. they wanted to avoid the situation. i think it was also because they didn 't want to take time to listen to me. it was better for them to pretend, to say nothing. (sagren) the actions of parents and significant others also shaped how participants negotiated stuttering. parents and significant others were often in conflicting states between accepting them for who they are as pws and yet wanting to make them "right" or normal for society. the first set of strategies involved correcting the stutter via "lay" or professional strategies. a second set of strategies was disavowal, which occurred at school and/or at home. there was a "silent" acknowledgement of the problems associated with stuttering but these were not openly discussed. participants explained this as a cultural phenomenon in south africa during a historical period when it was uncommon for parents or teachers to talk to children about their (the children's) problems. they contrasted disavowal with the more recent and current situation in south africa (2000 to 2004), where the emphasis had shifted to disclosing and talking openly about problems. the value of disavowal was interpreted differently by participants in different contexts i.e. it was both useful and not useful. sagren and kumari suggested that the lack of direct support at home fostered a greater sense of vulnerability and disempowerment, contributing to their disotherness. in the school environment, some teachers also used a strategy of disavowal that participants responded to differently. sagren, for example, suggested that teachers excluded him from having to deliver oral speeches that would have been difficult (for him and them) to manage. he also suspected that disavowal might have been used as a strategy by those teachers who did not want to take the time to listen to him which served to isolate him and reinforce his self-identity as disother. for others (kumari, gareth, hennie and thabo), disavowal drew attention away from the problem, allowing everyday routines to continue. society's penalties for stuttering, evident here as a cloak of silence, can be harsh and intolerant (ross & deverell, 2004) and contributed to reinforcing a self-identity as disother. implications, recommendations and limitations this study has illustrated the potential of life history research to generate knowledge of a personal nature. the consequence of introducing self-identity into the research dialogue is that it breathes life and people into research thereby admitting subjectivity as a cornerstone of knowledge production. life history methodology extends personal focus by highlighting the importance of understanding temporal and social dimensions of experience therefore contributing to understanding the "wholeness" of experience. the individual stories illuminate the variation and fluidity in the formation of self-identities and offers alternative interpretation to linear, stage model theories of stuttering development that have long been challenged by van riper. he contested the inadequacy of sectioning and categorising data and suggested "when adequate longitudinal data are available the concept of phases or stages will be completely discarded. human beings have ways of slipping through meshes of all categories. we are tired of wielding empty nets" (van riper, 1982, p. 92). biographical research aspires to address this concern and it is recommended that research of this nature be expanded. this study has produced knowledge of personal dimensions and therefore may have relevance and application for clinicians in many settings as they deal with the complex and multiple realities of their clients' lives. it is recommended that clinicians extend their roles beyond that of technicians (ross & deverell, 2004) and connect with the richly woven fabric of self-identity, social realities, value systems, emotions, family circumstances, personal choices and events in their clients' lives. to this end, engagement with the complex lives of pws are emphasised in narrative-based interventions (dilollo, niemeyer & manning, 2002). moreover, this study revealed that despite the multiple biographical contexts and experiences of participants, the genesis of the self-identity as disother was robust for all participants. society's role in creating disability must receive critical consideration. societies which value "order" (marotta, 2002) and "normality" contribute to shaping how pws are constructed and responded to as other. disability activists barnes, mercer and shakespeare (1999) have therefore insisted that impairment in itself does not produce disability. society contributes to producing disability by constructing and texturising issues of impairment within a discourse of negativity. therefore, interventions at a societal level are recommended. snyder (2000) has argued that unless societies are more accommodating and understanding of pws, professional interventions will fail to make a significant difference in their lives. j as an important first step to addressing the social processes contributing to disability it is recommended that the professions' knowledge base is interrogated to uncover its' inherent prejudices and dominant stereotypical attitudes towards people who stutter. the theoretical base of the profession is grounded within in a deficit medical model (pillay, 2003), and it has been established that speech pathologists have negative stereotypes of pws which are resistant to change (snyder, 2001). therefore, changing professional attitudes may be an important first step. further to this the role of schools, media and general public in creating disability must also be addressed. the actions taken by participants have traditionally been understood as secondary aspects/behaviours of stuttering (guitar, 1998). in this study actions emerged as identity management strategies. all people who stutter will live die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) is of self-identity as disother: life histories of people who stutter 13 differently by virtue of their historical, cultural, social and personal contexts and they act in varied ways in the interests of their identity management. therefore, clinicians are encouraged to understand the actions of pws in relation to their self-identities. the unfolding self-identity trajectories in some stories demonstrate that understanding difference may occur early in life. although the dominant literature on the young child who stutters does not routinely address issues of emotions and attitudes (guitar, 1998), the results of this study point to participants' vulnerability and the pervasive effects of traumatic early experiences. interventions should be cognisant of early processes shaping self-identity formation and consider the appropriate nature of intervention with young children. in addition the issues of race, power and life circumstances must be foregrounded in understanding and intervening with complex lives. a significant limitation of the study was that the researcher was unable to interview additional participants due to time and human resource constraints. for example, it would have been useful to interview participants who had been reared in rural areas and who those who had no access to formal education. there were some pws who met some criteria for selection but were unwilling to share their stories because they did not know or trust the researcher. in this regard, life history methodology is restrictive because it can only access understanding of those who are willing to share stories. a further limitation was that was that participants did not participate fully in the second level analytical process and therefore the analysis was generated primarily by the researcher. a comprehensive review of limitations of the study is available (kathard, 2003). conclusion this study explored the processes of the self-identity formation as disother and the actions of participants who stutter using a biographical, narrative life history methodology. the findings indicated that a complex set of personal and social processes operate in shaping self-identity as disother over time. the initial shaping of sejf-identity as disother began with participants' understanding themselves as different through a variety of critical events. multiple social processes in their immediate home, school and work contexts reinforced their self-identities as disother. the personal, bodily experience of stuttering coupled with processes of self-judging, resulted in participants making a critical transition from discovering themselves as different to attaching meaning to themselves as "less than". the actions they took were context-dependent and served in the interests of their identity management. there was unevenness in how the trajectory of disother unfolded for each participant over time and the process was influenced by their biographical circumstances. the study makes a case for developing personal and experiential knowledge on stuttering. it has considered the implications, recommendations and limitations of the study. acknowledgements the participants in the | study for sharing their stories so generously. the national research foundation, for supporting the doctoral study (2001-2003) through the part-time doctoral scholarship programme, social sciences. references angen, m. j. (2000). evaluating interpretive inquiry: reviewing the validity debate and opening the dialogue. qualitative health research, 10, 378-395. barnes, c., mercer, g. & shakespeare, t. (1999). exploring disability. united kingdom: polity press. bloodstein, o. (1995). handbook of stuttering. (5th ed.). london: singular publishing group. boehmer, e. (1995). colonial and postcolonial literature. oxford: oxford university press. castells, m. (1997). the power of identity. oxford: blackwell. charmaz, k. (1995). grounded theory. in j. a. smith, r. harre, & l. van langenhove (eds.), rethinking methods in psychology (pp. 2749). london: sage publications. clandinin, d. j. & connelly, ε μ. (2000). narrative inquiry. experience and story in qualitative research. san francisco: jossey-bass publishers. corcoran, j. a. & stewart, m. (1998). stories of stuttering. a qualitative analysis of interview narratives. journal of fluency disorders, 23, 247-264. creswell, j. w. & miller, d. l. (2000). determining validity in qualitative inquiry. theory into practice, 39,124-130. crichton-smith, i. (2002). communicating in the real-world: accounts from people who stammer. journal of fluency disorders, 27, 333-352. dilollo, α., manning, w. & neimeyer, r. (2002). a personal construct psychology view of relapse: indications for a narrative component to stuttering treatment. journal of fluency disorders, 27, 19-42. fine, m. (1998). working the hyphens: reinventing self and other in qualitative research. in n.k. denzin, & y.s. lincoln (eds.), the landscape of qualitative research. theories and issues (pp.139-155). london: sage publications. foucault, m. (1977). discipline and punish. london: allen lane. frank, a.w. (2001) can we research suffering? qualitative health research, 11, 353-362. frank, a.w. (2002). narrative analysis. why study people's stories? the dialogical ethics of narrative analysis. international journal of qualitative methods http://www.ualberta.ca/~ijqm/ downloaded 19 october 2002. freeman, d. (1996). to take them at their word: language data in the study of teachers' knowledge. harvard educational review, 66, 732-761. goffman, e. (1963). stigma. englewood-cliffs, nj: prenticehall green, t. (1999). the relationship of self-conception to perceived stuttering severity in children and adults who stutter. journal of fluency disorders, 24, 281-292. guba, e.g. & lincoln, y.s. (1998). competing paradigms in qualitative research. in n.k. denzin, & y.s. lincoln (eds.), the landscape of qualitative research (pp. 195220). thousand oaks, ca: sage. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) http://www.ualberta.ca/~ijqm/ 14 harsha kathard, mershen pillay, michael samuel and vijay reddy guitar, b. (1998). stuttering. an integrated approach to its nature and treatment (2nd ed.). london: williams & wilkins. harry, b. (1996). these families, those families. the impact of researcher identities on the research act. exceptional children, 62, 292-300. hatch, j.a. & wisniewski, r. (1995). life history and narrative: questions, issues and exemplary works. in j. a. hatch, & r. wisniewski (eds.), life history and narrative. london: falmer press. hayhow, r., cray, a. & enderbury, r (2002). stammering and therapy views of people who stammer. journal of fluency disorders, 27,1-17. howard, j.a. (2000), social psychology of identities. annual review of sociology, 26, 367-393. josselson, r. (1995), imagining the real: empathy, narrative and the dialogic self. in r. josselson, & a. lieblich (eds.), interpreting experience. the narrative study of lives (pp. 27-44). london: sage publications. kalinowski, l. s., lerman, j. w. & watt, j. (1987). a preliminary investigation of perception of self and others in stutterers and nonstutterers. journal of fluency disorders, 12, 317-331. kathard, h. (2001). sharing stories: life history narratives in stuttering research, international journal of language and communication disorders, 36, 52-57. kathard, h. (2003). life histories of people who stutter. on becoming someone. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of durban-westville, kwazulu natal, south africa. kvale, s. (1996). interviews: an introduction to qualitative research interviewing. thousand oaks, ca: sage lincoln, y.s. & guba, e.g. (1985). naturalistic inquiry. california: sage. marotta, v. (2002). zygmunt bauman: order, strangerhood and freedom, thesis eleven, 7, 36-54. measor, l. & sikes, r (1992). visiting lives. ethics and methodology in life history. in i. ε goodson (ed.), studying teachers' lives, (pp. 209-233). new york: teachers college press. mishler, e.g. (1999). storylines. craftartists narratives of identity. london: harvard university press. mowrer, d.e. (1998) alternative research strategies for the investigation of stuttering. journal of fluency disorders, 23, 89-97. perry, j. (1996). writing the self. exploring the stigma of hearing impairment. sociological spectrum, 16,239-261. perkins, w.h. (1997). stuttering: why science hasn't solved it. in r.e curlee, & g.m. siegel (eds.), nature and treatment of stuttering. new directions (2nd ed., pp. 218-234). boston: allyn and bacon. peshkin, a. (2001). angles of vision: enhancing perception in qualitative research, qualitative inquiry, 7, 238-253. petrunik, m. & shearing, c.d. (1983). fragile fa9ade: stuttering and the strategic manipulation of awareness. social problems, 31, 125-136. pillay, m. (2003). (re) positioning the powerful expert and the sick person: the case of communication pathology. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of durban-westville, kwazulu natal, south africa. plununer, k. (2001). documents of life 2. an invitation to critical humanism. london: sage publications. polkinghorne, d.e. (1996). explorations of narrative identity. psychological inquiry, 1, 363-367. quesal, r.w. (1989). stuttering research. have we forgotten the stutterer? journal of fluency disorders, 14, 153-164. reddy, v. (2000). life histories of black south african scientists: academic success in an unequal society. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of durban-westville, kwazulu natal, south africa. ross, e. & deverell, a. (2004). psychosocial approaches to health, illness and disability. pretoria: van schaik publishers. rubin, η j., & rubin, i. (1995). qualitative interviewing. the art of hearing data. london: sage publications. sacks, o. (1995). an anthropologist on mars: seven paradoxical tales. picador: london. samuel, m. (1998). words, lives and music. on becoming a teacher of english. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of durban-westville, kwazulu natal, south africa. silverman, e. (2001, october). consumer alert: stuttering and gender research. paper presented at fourth international stuttering awareness day conference. retrieved october 2001 from http://www.mnsu.edu/dept/comradis/isad4/ papers/silverman2 snyder, g.j. (2000). an analysis of stuttering treatment measurements and therapy efficacy: implications for a new stuttering therapy paradigm. retrieved may 17 2001 from http://www.stuttering.advocate.net/paper.html snyder, g.j. (2001). exploratory research in the measurement and modification of attitudes toward stuttering. journal of fluency disorders, 26, 149-160. somers, m.r. (1994). the narrative constitution of identity: a relational and network approach. theory and society. renewal and critique in social theory, 23, 604-635. sue-o'brien, d. (1993). stuttering. dimensions of an unfolding experience. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of southern california, los angeles. i valsiner, j. (2002). forms of dialogical relations and semiotic autoregulations within the self. theory & psychology', 12, 251-265. i van riper, c. (1982). the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, nj: prentice hall. yaruss, j., quesal, r.w., reeves, l., molt, l.r., kluetz, b., caruso, a.j., mcclure, j.a. & lewis, f. (2002). speech treatment and support group experiences of people who participate in the national stuttering association. journal of fluency disorders, 27, 115-134. yovetich, w.m.s., leschied, a.w. & flicht, j. (2000). selfesteem of school-age children who stutter/journal of fluency disorders, 25,143-153. zelen, s.l., sheehan, j.g. & bugenthal, j.f.t. (1954). selfperceptions in stuttering. journal of clinical psychology, 10, 70-72. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) http://www.mnsu.edu/dept/comradis/isad4/ http://www.stuttering.advocate.net/paper.html real partner helps you reach l i r i l 9 5 3 , t w o y o u n g m e n w o r k e d t o g e t h e r t o c o n q u e r o n e o f m a n k i n d ' s final f r o n t i e r s m t . e v e r e s t . it's t h e s o r t o f a c h i e v e m e n t t h a t c a n o n l y c o m e f r o m a g r e a t p a r t n e r s h i p . a t w i d e x w e a r e d e d i c a t e d t o b u i l d i n g r e l a t i o n s h i p s t h a t b r e a k n e w f r o n t i e r s in m a n a g i n g h e a r i n g loss. since t h e l a u n c h o f t h e w i d e x c h a l l e n g e in 2 0 0 3 , w e h a v e s u p p o r t e d six audiologists in e x p l o r i n g u n c h a r t e r e d t e r r i t o r y t o shift local i g n o r a n c e a b o u t h e a r i n g loss. o u r p a r t n e r s h i p s w i t h a u d i o l o g i s t s r e v e a l e d : • n e w insights f r o m u r b a n i n f o r m a l s e t t l e m e n t s w i t h r e g a r d t o t h e a w a r e n e s s o f d e a l i n g w i t h h e a r i n g loss • t h e p o t e n t i a l r o l e o f r u r a l kgotlas w i t h e d u c a t i n g local c o m m u n i t i e s a b o u t h e a r i n g loss • c u r r e n t a t t i t u d e s a n d b e h a v i o u r s w i t h i n c o r p o r a t e e n v i r o n m e n t s w i t h r e g a r d t o d e a l i n g w i t h h e a r i n g loss » r e l e v a n t g u i d e l i n e s t o assist p a r e n t s w h o h a v e c h i l d r e n w i t h h e a r i n g loss a l l t h e s e d i s c o v e r i e s a n d insights i n t o o u r local c o m m u n i t y w i l l in s o m e small w a y c o n t r i b u t e t o shifting t h e h u r d l e o f i g n o r a n c e a n d f e a r a b o u t h e a r i n g loss. b y w o r k i n g t o g e t h e r a n d s h a r i n g d i s c o v e r i e s w e c a n r e a c h n e w h e i g h t s a n d a c h i e v e m o r e in c o n q u e r i n g p u b l i c i g n o r a n c e a b o u t h e a r i n g loss. if y o u a r e i n t e r e s t e d in c o n q u e r i n g t h e c h a l l e n g e o f p u b l i c i g n o r a n c e a b o u t h e a r i n g loss w i t h s p e c i a l i s e d , social r e s e a r c h p r o j e c t s , p l e a s e c o n t a c t pk n a g i n o r b r e n d a n m c g u i r k at w i d e x . w e w i l l r e v i e w y o u r a p p l i c a t i o n w i t h o u r 2 0 0 5 i n t a k e o f a p p l i c a t i o n s . if y o u call us at ( 0 3 1 ) 5 6 3 4 4 2 5 w e w i l l also s h a r e o u r d i s c o v e r i e s o v e r t h e past t w o y e a r s w i t h y o u . v j j i d e x hear the difference r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pg13-18.html the development of a neonatal communication intervention tool esedra strasheim alta kritzinger department of communication pathology, university of pretoria brenda louw department of audiology and speech-language pathology, east tennessee state university correspondence to: e strasheim (esedra1@gmail.com) abstract neonatal communication intervention is important in south africa, which has an increased prevalence of infants born with risks for disabilities and where the majority of infants live in poverty. local literature showed a dearth of information on the current service delivery and roles of speech-language therapists (slts) and audiologists in neonatal nurseries in the south african context. slts have the opportunity to provide the earliest intervention, provided that intervention is well-timed in the neonatal nursery context. the aim of the research was to compile a locally relevant neonatal communication intervention instrument/tool for use by slts in neonatal nurseries of public hospitals. the study entailed descriptive, exploratory research. during phase 1, a survey was received from 39 slts and 2 audiologists in six provinces. the data revealed that participants performed different roles in neonatal nurseries, which depended on the environment, tools, materials and instrumentation available to them. many participants were inexperienced, but resourceful in their attempts to adapt tools/materials. participants expressed needs for culturally appropriate and user-friendly instruments for parent guidance and staff/team training on the topic of developmental care. during phase 2, a tool for parent guidance titled neonatal communication intervention programme for parents was compiled in english and isizulu. the programme was piloted by three participants. suggestions for enhancements of the programme were made, such as providing a glossary of terms, adapting the programme’s language and terminology, and providing more illustrations. slts and audiologists must contribute to neonatal care of high-risk infants to facilitate development and to support families. keywords: developmental care, early communication intervention, neonatal communication intervention, neonatal nursery, public health context comprehensive management in the neonatal nursery includes not only medical treatment of the infant but also developmental care and the provision of guidance, counselling and information to the family who are part of the decision-making process regarding the infant’s care (asha, 2005). a programme that has shown positive results for premature infants is the newborn individual developmental care and assessment programme (nidcap) (als et al., 2004). this programme requires specialised training based in the usa, which makes it inaccessible to most south african professionals. kangaroo mother care (kmc) has been shown to be a safe alternative for third-world countries, where 96% of the world’s premature infants are born (bergman, linley & fawcus, 2004). the effectiveness and safety of kmc is well established (bergman, malan & hann, 2003) and it is regarded as an important developmental care practice for developing, as well as developed, contexts (bergman et al., 2004). in south africa’s public health sector, early communication intervention (eci) services to neonates are less developed and less comprehensive in comparison with those of a developed country such as the usa (kritzinger, louw & hugo, 1995). while the role of the speech-language therapist (slt) in the neonatal intensive care unit (nicu) is clearly described in international literature (asha, 2005; rossetti, 2001; ziev, 1999), currently no guidelines for service delivery in the nicu in the south african context exist (de beer, 2003). slts who are employed in south african provincial hospitals are often faced with difficult working conditions, such as lack of community awareness of services, inadequate instrumentation and tools, insufficient services of trained interpreters and limited literacy of caregivers (fair & louw, 1999). the diversity of language and culture in south africa poses a challenge for slts in providing family-centred early intervention services (louw & avenant, 2002). current health care policies in south africa prioritise care of mothers and young children, as can be seen in legislation that emphasises the provision of free primary health care to children under the age of 6 years (national health act, 2003). although eci may be regarded as a component of services to women and young children, it is still not a health care priority in south africa. the undervalued perception of eci is not only due to the hiv/aids pandemic, but also to the limited knowledge about the benefits of eci, a shortage of eci facilities and early communication interventionists, an insufficient referral system and poor teamwork (kritzinger, 2000). in a study conducted by h. louw (2007) regarding eci service delivery in public hospitals in four provinces in south africa, it was concluded that some of the high-risk infants and families were still not receiving linguistically appropriate services. a shortage of qualified and trained interpreters was also identified (h. louw, 2007), which presents an obstacle to effective eci service delivery in public hospitals. south africa has 11 official languages, of which isizulu is the most commonly spoken (24%) with english in 5th place (8%) (population census key results, 2001); english is the language most commonly spoken by slts. language differences may pose a considerable barrier to effective understanding between professionals and families (madding, 2000). h. louw (2007) found that only 11% of the respondents in her study worked with trained interpreters at their hospitals. since the multilingual nature of the south african population creates barriers to service delivery, it necessitates different approaches to intervention. the shortage of slts who can provide eci and initiate prevention campaigns (fair & louw, 1999) and the insufficient number of therapists in the public hospital context result in large caseloads. another challenge to eci in south africa is a dearth of apparatus and materials for the assessment and treatment of high-risk and at-risk infants. h. louw’s findings (2007) indicated that 93% of community service slts employed in mpumalanga, western cape, kwazulu-natal and gauteng in the public health sector expressed the need for more culturally and language-appropriate materials specifically designed to address the unique needs of the south african community. the results confirm the earlier findings of kritzinger et al. (1995) that there are limited diagnostic tools developed from a speech-language pathology and audiology perspective for neonatal assessment and management. in south africa, poverty is a characteristic of the majority of the neonatal population requiring eci. poverty in itself may not be the direct cause of developmental problems in young children, but family conditions such as malnutrition, inadequate prenatal care, exposure to infectious diseases and toxicants in utero, unsafe living conditions, living with parents who are addicted to alcohol or drugs and inadequate educational opportunities are all common in circumstances of poverty (thompson, 1992). certain risk conditions associated with communication disorders, such as low birth weight, cerebral palsy, fetal alcohol spectrum disorder and hiv/aids, also have a higher prevalence in south africa than in developed countries (kritzinger, 2000; swanepoel, 2004). the high prevalence of conditions affecting young children is of significance to slts, as these conditions can be related to many developmental disorders but more specifically to communication delays and disorders (kritzinger, 2000; rossetti, 2001). asha (2004) states that slts have the responsibility to fulfil eci roles using practices that are based on research, family-centred, culturally and linguistically appropriate, developmentally appropriate and collaborative. the execution of these responsibilities and functions is dependent upon a well-developed theoretical and clinical foundation. evidence-based practice is a framework for clinical decision making that entails the integration of best research evidence with clinical expertise and patient values (johnson, 2006). according to louw (2007), basing clinical decisions on scientific research is fundamental to ethical practice in eci. in order to fulfil the roles and responsibilities in neonatal communication intervention effectively, the slt requires certain tools. local research already provides guidelines to useful training strategies. gani (2004) found materials based on the hanen programme (pepper & weitzman, 2004) to be useful in training a group of caregivers in communication stimulation in a care centre, and a positive effect on caregiver-child interaction patterns was determined. best practice in developmental care in neonatal nurseries needs to be encouraged and facilitated, which necessitates the development of appropriate tools for the local context. asha (2005) urges speech-language pathologists to develop culturally appropriate programmes that meet the needs of ethnically and linguistically diverse families. culturally and contextually appropriate tools and programmes for neonatal communication intervention therefore need to be developed for the south african context, in order to serve the unique high-risk population in south africa in an effective and ethical manner. the unique contextual reality of south africa should be taken into account. to be contextually relevant, tools should be based upon an expressed need within a given context. if based on western models of communication stimulation, these tools should be adapted appropriately for the south african context. because of the diversity of cultural groups in south africa, overseas tools and programmes cannot merely be translated (visser, 2005). the interdependent relationship between culture and language must be carefully considered during cross-cultural service delivery, otherwise the tool will still not be suitable even once translated (pakendorf, 1998). it may also be beneficial to consider other adaptations in order to bridge additional challenges such as limited literacy among caregivers. according to louw, shibambu and roemer (2006), literacy issues may be overcome by using visual sources such as pictorial illustrations and demonstrations, as written materials are not necessarily viewed as important, especially by individuals with low literacy levels. to propose a solution to address the dearth of tools in the public health context, a survey was conducted to determine the perceptions of slts and audiologists providing services in provincial hospitals in south africa. the aim of the survey was to investigate the role of slts in the neonatal nurseries and to identify participants’ needs in terms of neonatal communication intervention instruments/tools. this information was utilised to compile a preliminary instrument/tool based on the selection of one of the perceived needs of the participants. method aim the overall aim of this study was to compile a locally relevant neonatal communication intervention instrument/tool for use by slts in the neonatal nurseries of public hospitals. the following objectives were formulated in order to reach the main aim: • to describe the perceptions of slts and audiologists providing eci services in provincial hospitals in south africa regarding their role in the neonatal nurseries • to identify participants’ needs in terms of neonatal communication intervention instruments/tools • to select and justify a specific need of the participants in terms of neonatal communication intervention instruments/tools in the public hospital context • to compile a preliminary instrument/tool based on the selection of one of the perceived needs of the participants • to pre-test the completed instrument/tool and make changes, if necessary. research design for the purpose of this study a descriptive, exploratory study within the quantitative and qualitative frameworks of research design was selected. ethical considerations ethical clearance to conduct the study was obtained from the research ethics committee of the faculty of humanities, university of pretoria. all participants gave written informed consent. data were treated with confidentiality and no identifying information of the participants or their hospitals was reported. participants all the participants in this study had to provide eci to infants in a neonatal nursery such as an nicu, a neonatal high-care ward or a kmc ward, as this study specifically focused on the participants’ needs regarding neonatal communication intervention. permission was obtained in writing from 6 of the 9 provinces contacted, namely gauteng, kwazulu-natal, eastern cape, north west, northern cape and mpumalanga. the departments of health of the free state, western cape and limpopo provinces did not respond within the time constraints of this study, which further reduced the population available for the research. the provincial health departments were requested to provide statistics regarding the number of slts and audiologists employed at the hospitals in their province, as well as to provide each hospital’s contact details. the potential participants targeted for the study were contacted telephonically at the hospitals to explain the aim of the study and discuss participation. all participants who responded were included in the study. questionnaires were sent to 175 slts and audiologists in public hospitals in the six provinces. a total of 41 slts and audiologists returned a completed questionnaire, a return rate of 23%. the responses obtained are therefore not representative of speech-language therapy and audiology services in the public health sector in south africa. the majority of participants worked mostly at district or regional hospitals, as well as at community outreach clinics. they mostly provided eci in neonatal high-care units and kmc units of their hospitals. most participants appeared to be inexperienced in providing neonatal communication intervention services. they may have been without supervision, as most participants were the only slts/audiologists employed at their hospital and had limited access to interpreters and assistants. the participants’ characteristics are displayed in table i. research process the research was conducted in two sequential phases. data collection: phase 1 a self-designed questionnaire was selected as the data collection tool to conduct the survey. a pilot study was conducted to pre-test the questionnaire which provided recommendations to enhance its efficacy and practicality. the questionnaire was e-mailed or faxed to the potential participants during phase 1 of the study. the first section gathered biographical data, which was used to describe the participants and to interpret the data from other sections of the questionnaire. the following section enquired about the service delivery regarding eci in the hospital, task allocations, contexts for eci (nicu, neonatal high care, kmc) and the participants’ functions and roles in terms of assessment and intervention with the infant, parents or staff. the questionnaire enquired about the participants’ needs in terms of service delivery, their perceptions of culturally appropriate and user-friendly tools, and their needs in terms of tools regarding assessment, intervention directed at the parents/caregivers and intervention directed at the staff/team. data analysis: phase 1 frequency distribution was set up from the raw data to obtain an overall view of the data (maxwell & satake, 2006). descriptive statistics were used to examine the data and to graphically display the data (maxwell & satake, 2006). the qualitative data were presented as detailed textual descriptions, and phase 1 included direct quotations from participants (fossey, harvey, mcdermott & davidson, 2002). method: phase 2 the needs analysis in phase 1 informed the type and the format of the tool, which was compiled during phase 2. the tool could therefore only be compiled after the results of phase 1 were obtained. for this reason, the instrument/tool was pre-tested in a pilot study as part of phase 2 and presented in the results. the evaluation of the training tool entailed descriptive data, which were analysed according to recurring themes. validity in this study slts’ and audiologists’ perceptions of their roles, their competence, work satisfaction and their needs were constructs that could not be measured directly. the construct validity could therefore be influenced by the participants’ subjective opinions and by the wording of questions in the questionnaire. the content of the questionnaire was reviewed by a statistician to determine whether the questions were relevant and appropriate for statistical purposes. the construct and content validity of the questionnaire were determined by making use of a pilot study, and certain changes were implemented according to the recommendations made. because of the small sample size, the results of this survey were not representative of the perceptions of all slts and audiologists employed in government hospitals. therefore no attempt will be made to generalise the findings. reliability the questionnaire was piloted to determine whether any items were misleading or unclear, which could result in participants misinterpreting some items. the pilot study therefore also contributed to the reliability of the questionnaire. the data collection procedures were described in detail, contributing to the repeatability of the study and thereby increasing its reliability. results and discussion: phase 1 participants’ roles in the neonatal nursery figure 1 illustrates that some participants performed roles in the nicu that require specialised equipment, such as hearing evaluation and video-fluoroscopy. video-fluoroscopic instrumentation is not readily available at all hospitals and is usually only found in tertiary or academic hospitals because of the costs involved, which explains why few participants performed this role. this finding can be explained by the fact that most of the participants were employed at district or regional hospitals that would not have specialised equipment such as video-fluoroscopy. participants appeared to be performing multidimensional professional roles relating to screening and assessment, which is attributed to their awareness of the risks of development problems in the areas of attachment and communication development in infants with low birth weight and preterm birth. the majority of the participants reported that they fulfil a number of roles in direct intervention with infants and parents, as seen in figure 2. most slts were the only slt/audiologist employed in their department and had large caseloads, which possibly did not leave time to engage in discharge planning and planning of follow-up services. because patients come from a wide geographical area and have limited finances and transport to return to the hospital or clinic, regular follow-up services are a problem area in the south african public health sector (fair & louw, 1999). given the nature of the south african context, the roles performed by the participants need to be expanded and adapted to meet local needs to improve neonatal communication intervention service delivery. according to figure 3, most participants appeared to be aware of the impact they might have in this context through teamwork, as this is preferred practice because of the benefits for the infant, the family and the slt. early intervention in the nicu should follow a transdisciplinary team approach (rossetti, 2001). however, in-service training of staff members was not yet performed by all facilities, and their service delivery was limited by poor attendance of ward rounds. ziev (1999) describes this function as an opportunity to learn from others and to become a familiar face among team members, as well as to request referrals based on infants’ symptoms or histories. as mentioned earlier, many participants were the only slt/audiologist employed in their department, which may have limited the time available for them to attend neonatal ward rounds as they may have been involved in other paediatric or adult ward rounds, clinics or consultations. this is problematic as many participants had limited experience, which resulted in missed opportunities in the neonatal nursery. two of the participants in this study were qualified as audiologists only. only one of the two audiologists performed hearing screenings, which is ascribed to the fact that hearing screening equipment is not readily available in government hospitals (theunissen & swanepoel, 2008). both audiologists worked as part of a team and consulted with other team members in accordance with asha’s guidelines for audiologists (2008b). current literature emphasises the importance of ongoing audiological and medical monitoring of any child who demonstrates risk indicators for delayed onset or progressive hearing loss for at least 3 years (northern & downs, 2002). interestingly, both audiologists also assisted in discharge planning and planning of follow-up treatment/management after discharge, while only half of the larger group of 39 slt participants performed this role. this may be due to a lack of infant follow-up clinics at many district or regional hospitals. families need information, consistent encouragement, reassurance, and positive feedback regarding their competency and ability to cope with the birth and hospitalisation of their critically ill newborn (northern & downs, 2002). participants’ needs in the neonatal nursery an overview of the needs of the slts as determined in phase 1 was compiled in table ii. the 5 most frequently indicated needs are highlighted, namely tools for parent guidance (4 topics), a tool for staff and team training (1 topic) and an assessment tool (1 topic) (2 needs in 4th place). eci services are required to be family-centred, as well as culturally linguistically responsive (asha, 2008a). neonatal communication intervention services therefore cannot be effective without culturally appropriate tools. an urgent need for culturally appropriate materials for use in the neonatal nurseries of provincial hospitals was identified. participants were aware of the importance of providing culturally sensitive services, but were hampered by the dearth of tools and materials that could be utilised in clinical practice. phase 1 demonstrated that participants performed different roles in neonatal nurseries, which were determined by the environment, tools, materials and instrumentation available to them. although many participants were relatively inexperienced, they were resourceful in their attempts to develop and adapt tools and materials. the fact that these self-developed and adapted tools are not research-based compromises the quality of services and precludes best practice. the participants expressed a need for culturally appropriate and user-friendly instruments specifically for parent guidance and staff/team training. these descriptive findings justified the compilation and development of a locally relevant instrument/tool for use in public hospitals’ neonatal nurseries. results and discussion: phase 2 the compilation of a neonatal communication intervention tool a tool regarding parent guidance with the topic of developmental care was selected as the tool to be compiled, as this was a need indicated by the majority of the slt participants. most participants were also involved in services directed at parents/caregivers in the neonatal nursery. the ‘neonatal communication intervention programme for parents’ aimed to provide slts in local public hospitals with a programme to educate and guide parents/caregivers of infants in the neonatal nursery regarding developmental care, early reciprocal interaction and appropriate stimulation of their infants. adult-learning principles were utilised in the compilation of the programme. guidelines to teach adult learners consider mature learners as independent and self-directed (kaufman, 2003). the researcher formulated an aim as well as outcomes for the programme (popich, 2003). the structure and sequence of the programme as conceptualised is illustrated in figure 4. the programme is divided into four sections, which include an introduction with ‘warm-up time’ and definitions, information on the behaviours of the neonate, information on how parents may respond to these behaviours, and a conclusion that informs parents of options for follow-up services and provides time for reflection and questions. informal terminology was used to make the language more accessible to the parents/caregivers. the term ‘baby’ is used in the programme instead of ‘infant’ as this is the term most commonly used by south african parents in conversation (popich, 2003). it was decided that the programme should consist of a microsoft powerpoint presentation and a handout for parents. the powerpoint presentation was provided in two different formats as it allows each clinician to select a method that is conducive to his/her working environment. some slts and audiologists in public hospitals have limited technical resources, and the powerpoint presentation was therefore provided on a compact disc as well as transparencies. handouts were provided to parents during the programme to generalise and reinforce the newly learnt information as well as to actively involve each parent during the presentation. the handout included photos and images to manage literacy barriers. the content of the handout was translated into isizulu by a private translation service to address language barriers. isizulu was selected as it is the home language of almost a quarter of south africans and is the language most spoken in south africa (population census key results, 2001). the programme was pre-tested in a pilot study, which concluded that the programme was enjoyed by the parents who received the training. it was further determined that the content was appropriate but that the programme should be more concise and shorter so as to be more user-friendly. the parent handout was deemed suitable for the training. certain suggestions for enhancements of the programme were made during the pilot study, such as providing a glossary of terms with definitions for therapists to use and adapting the programme’s language and terminology. clinical and theoretical implications the current research identified and attempted to fulfil a need expressed by slts and audiologists. the study managed to achieve the aim of compiling a programme for slts for use with parents and caregivers in the neonatal nurseries in south african provincial hospitals. this study highlighted the role of the slt and audiologist in terms of prevention of communication delays and disorders. this research is an example for slts and audiologists in the public health sector on how to use adult education principles in parent training to prevent communication delays and disorders in infants. the programme could also be used for raising awareness of eci services within a certain community. south africa has an increased prevalence of infants at risk for communication disorders (kritzinger, 2000), and marketing of eci among the general public, as well as health care workers, should therefore be a priority of local slts working in the public health sector. according to asha (2008a) eci services must promote children’s participation in natural environments, which include community settings outside the home environment, where children without disabilities participate. community work increases the existing professional knowledge on diverse communities within south africa, which presents therapists with the opportunity to implement prevention programmes such as adult training (popich, 2003). this study emphasised involvement in community work and not only in the lives of individual families. the research could be used as an example of caregiver training within a specific community in order to reach more infants and toddlers in need of eci services and to improve the current services in communities. this study highlighted information on the roles of slts and audiologists in the neonatal care of high-risk infants in the public health sector. this research gathered valuable information regarding the roles and responsibilities of slts and audiologists in local nicu, neonatal high-care and kmc nurseries. it can therefore be used to guide future attempts to compile local guidelines for slts in the nicu. conclusion the study complied with the guiding principles for best practice in eci (asha, 2008a) as it aims to be family-centred, culturally and linguistically responsive and developmentally supportive, to promote children’s participation in their natural environments, to be comprehensive, co-ordinated and team-based, and is based on evidence. this research therefore contributed to neonatal care of high-risk infants in south africa. the increased prevalence of infants at risk for communication disorders in south africa (kritzinger, 2000) necessitates early interventionists to become involved in clinical and research efforts to develop eci services for provincial hospitals. slts and audiologists not only have an essential role to fulfil in the neonatal nursery, but also have an ethical responsibility to develop creative solutions for challenges arising from service delivery in the south african public health context. slts and audiologists must contribute to neonatal care of high-risk infants to facilitate their optimal development. acknowledgements. the authors would like to thank the professionals who participated in this study and who are committed to early communication intervention. the corresponding author (esedra1@gmail.com) may be contacted for more information about obtaining the ‘neonatal communication intervention programme for parents’. authors’ note. since the article was accepted for publication, saslha has adopted revised guidelines for early communication intervention, which include specific guidelines on neonatal communication intervention. the document may be obtained from the ethics and standards committee (2011). guidelines: early communication intervention. www.saslha.co.za references als, h., duffy, f.h., mcanulty, g., rivkin, m.j., vajapeyam, s., mulkern, r.v., warfield, s.k., huppi, p.s., butler, s.c., conneman, n., fischer, c., & eichenwald, e.c. (2004). early experience alters brain function and structure. pediatrics, 113, 846-857. american speech-language hearing association (2004). roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists in the neonatal intensive care unit: position statement. retrieved on 24 april 2006 from www.asha.org american speech-language hearing association (2005). roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists in the neonatal intensive care unit: guidelines. retrieved on 24 april 2006 from http://www.asha.org american speech-language hearing association (2008a). roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists in early intervention: guidelines. retrieved on 27 august 2008 from http://www.asha.org american speech-language hearing association. (2008b). scope of practice in audiology. retrieved on 27 august 2008 from http://www.asha.org bergman, n.j., linley, l.l., & fawcus, s.r. (2004). randomized controlled trial of skin-to-skin contact from birth versus conventional incubator for physiological stabilization in 1200 2199 gram newborns. acta paediatrica, 93, 779-785. bergman, n.j., malan, a., & hann, m. (2003). news from the regions – south africa. fourth international workshop on kangaroo mother care. journal of tropical pediatrics, 49(5), 311-312. de beer, m. (2003). die rol van ’n groep spraak-taalterapeute in neonatale hoësorgeenhede, unpublished b. communication pathology research project, university of pretoria. fair, l., & louw, b. (1999). early communication intervention within a community-based intervention model in south africa. the south african journal of communication disorders, 46, 13-23. fossey, e., harvey, c., mcdermott, f., & davidson, l. (2002). understanding and evaluating qualitative research. australian and new zealand journal of psychiatry, 36, 717-732. gani, a. (2004). caregivers of infants and young children with paediatric hiv/aids in a care centre: impact of a training programme on caregiver-child-interaction patterns. unpublished b. communication pathology research report, university of pretoria. johnson, c. (2006). getting started in evidence-based practice for childhood speech-language disorders. american journal of speech-language pathology, 15, 20-35. kaufman, d.m. (2003). abc of learning and teaching in medicine: applying educational theory in practice. bmj, 326, 213-216. kritzinger, a.m. (2000). establishing a computer-based data system for early communication intervention in south africa. unpublished d.phil. thesis, faculty of humanities, university of pretoria. kritzinger, a., louw, b., & hugo, r. (1995). communication skills of biologically at-risk neonates. south african journal of communication disorders, 42, 7-17. louw, b. (2007). introduction to evidence-based practice in early communication intervention. powerpoint presentation at seminar on evidence based practice in early communication intervention, department of communication pathology, university of pretoria, august 2007. louw, b., & avenant, c. (2002). culture as a context for intervention: developing a culturally congruent early intervention programme. international paediatrics, 17(3), 145-150. louw, b., shibambu, m., & roemer, k. (2006). facilitating cleft palate team participation of culturally diverse families in south africa. cleft palate-craniofacial journal, 43(1), 47-54. louw, h. (2007). verskaffing en verspreiding van vroeë kommunikasie intervensie in die publieke gesondheidskonteks in geselekteerde provinsies in suid-afrika. unpublished b. communication pathology research report, department of communication pathology, university of pretoria. madding, c.c. (2000). maintaining focus on cultural competence in early intervention services to linguistically and culturally diverse families. infant-toddler intervention. the transdisciplinary journal, 10(1), 9-18. maxwell, d.l., & satake, e. (2006). research and statistical methods in communication disorders. london: williams & wilkins. national health act (2003). retrieved on 8 june 2007 from http://www.info.gov.za/speeches/2005/05042013451004.htm northern, j.l., & downs, m. (2002). hearing in children (5th ed.). baltimore: lippincott, williams & wilkins. pakendorf, c. (1998). 10 punt plan vir die vertaling en kulturele aanpassing van toetsmateriaal vir die suid-afrikaanse konteks. clinica: applications in clinical practice in communication pathology, monograph 3, 1-10. pepper, j., & weitzman, e. (2004). it takes two to talk. toronto: hanen centre. popich, e. (2003). the development of a tool for communication stimulation for infants (0 12 months). unpublished d.phil. thesis, faculty of humanities, university of pretoria. statistics south africa (2003). population census key results. retrieved on 8 june 2007 from http://www.statssa.gov.za/census01/html/key%20results_key%20results.pdf rossetti, l.m. (2001). communication intervention: birth to three (2nd ed). albany: singular thomson learning. swanepoel, d. (2004). a review of the south african context: information for community-based speech-language and hearing services. clinica: applications in clinical practice of communication pathology, monograph 9, 11-16. theunissen, m., & swanepoel, d. (2008). early hearing detection and intervention services in the public health sector in south africa. international journal of audiology, 47(1), 23-29. thompson, t. (1992). developmental risk factors and poverty. in t. thompson & s.c. hupp (eds.). saving children at risk: poverty and disabilities. newbury park: sage publications. visser, c. (2005). die toepaslikheid van ’n afrikaanse vertaling van die ‘scan-c: test for auditory processing disorders in children – revised’ vir voorskoolse leerders. unpublished m. communication pathology dissertation, university of pretoria. ziev, m.s.r. (1999). earliest intervention: speech-language pathology services in the neonatal intensive care unit. asha, may/june, 32-36. fig. 1. speech-language therapists’ indication of their roles regarding screening and assessment of the high-risk infant (n=39). fig. 2. speech-language therapists’ roles in intervention specifically directed at the infant and parents/caregivers (n=39). fig. 3. speech-language therapists’ roles in intervention specifically directed at staff and team members (n=39). fig. 4. structure and content of the programme. table i. characteristics of the participants professional qualifications slt – 49% slt & audiologist – 46% audiologist – 5% provinces where employed eastern cape – 5% gauteng – 8% kwazulu-natal – 25% mpumalanga – 29% northern cape – 28% north west – 5% years of experience in government sector 1 year or less – 25 participants 2 years – 6 participants 3 years – 3 participants 5 years or more – 6 participants contexts of service provision clinics/community health centres – 26 participants (participants indicated more than one working context) district/regional hospital – 30 participants tertiary/academic hospital – 12 participants other – 1 participant wards where ei was provided nicu – 14 participants neonatal high-care unit – 23 participants kangaroo mother care ward – 20 participants number of slps and/or audiologists in the department 1 – 15 participants 2 – 10 participants 3 5 participants 4 or more – 11 participants trained interpreters or assistants at their disposal yes – 10% no – 90% table ii. speech-language therapists’ needs (n=39) need no. of responses ( n =39) no. who indicated need % hierarchy 1 14 assessment tools neonate’s communication development 36 15 41.6 9 feeding 34 15 44.1 8 mother-child communication-interaction 33 16 48.4 7 neonatal nursery environment 33 19 57.5 3 parent guidance tools neonatal nursery environment and staff 36 24 66.6 1 paediatric dysphagia and feeding therapy 34 18 52.9 5 over-stimulation, identifying infant’s stress cues 35 19 54.2 4 developmental care 35 19 54.2 4 kangaroo mother care (kmc) 35 8 22.8 14 communication interaction with infant 35 12 34.2 11 developmental milestones and follow-up after discharge 34 9 26.4 12 normal communication development 36 9 25.0 13 staff/team training tools developmental care 36 23 63.8 2 kmc & eci 35 13 37.1 10 role of the slt in the neonatal nursery 36 19 52.7 6 highlighted sections indicate the most frequently mentioned needs. 59 a new look at cochlear mechanics carole a jardine department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract the spectacular discovery of otoacoustic emissions has led to a plethora of cochlear mechanic models, all attempting to explain the active, nonlinear processing of the cochlea suggested by these recordable responses. these hypothetical proposals have been largely based on animal experimentations, mathematically-based theorems, and observations in simulated environments. none have been irrevocably validated although there is much circumstantial evidence expounding their feasibility. advances in electron microscopy, mechanical engineering, histological examination techniques together with the technology enabling us to measure these emissions, have radically altered the current views on the assumptions of auditory mechanics. this paper briefly contrasts the previously established cochlear theories proposed by doyens such as helmholtz (1857) and von bikisy (1936) with current perspectives advanced by cell biologists and biophysicists. however, the exact nature of cochlear processing still remains a mystery. as numerous chasms of knowledge about audition are being filled, so even more questions are posed in a seemingly eternal quest for the answer! opsomming hierdie indrukwekkende ontdekking van otoakoustiese emissies het gelei tot 'n oormaat kogleere-werkingmodelle wat almal gepoog het om die aktiewe nie-lineere prosessering van die koglea wat deur optekenswaardige response voorgestel word, te verklaar. hierdie hipotetiese voorstelle is grotendeels op diere-eksperimente, wiskundig gefundeerde stellings en waarnemings in gesimuleerde omgewings gebaseer. nie een hiervan is onweerlegbaar bekragtig nie, alhoewel daar heelwat omstandigheidsgetuienis bestaan wat die uitvoerbaarheid daarvan verklaar. vooruitgang op die gebied van elektromikroskopie, meganiese ingenieurswese, histologiese ondersoektegnieke, tesame met die tegnologie wat die meting van hierdie emissies moontlik 'maak, het die algemene opvattings oor die aannames van ouditiewe funksionering radikaal verander. hierdie artikel vergelyk die voorheen opgestelde kogleere teoriee wat voorgestel is deur doyens soos helmholtz (1957) en von bekisy (1936) kortliks met die huidige perspektiewe wat deur bioloe en biofisici daargestel is. die presiese aard van prosessering bly egter'nog steeds 'n geheim. namate talryke leemtes in die kennis van gehoor nog aangevul word, word selfs meer vrae gestel in 'n oenskynlik nimmereindigende soeke na die antwoord! abbreviations used in text oae otoacoustic emissions eoae evoked otoacoustic emissions dpoae distortion product otoacoustic emissions bm basilar membrane ihc inner hair cell ohc outor hair cell rl reticular lamina ocn oliviocochlear nucleus atp adenosine triphosphate introduction "few areas of audiology have advanced as rapidly as cochlear physiology and biophysics have over the past decade. the advance began with the shock realisation that existing knowledge and accepted concepts could not explain the response of the cochlea to sound and in particular otoacoustic emisssions. our very understanding of both the physical basis of hearing and the nature of hearing impairment was challenged". (grandori, cianfrone and kemp, 1990, viii) as early as 1948, gold (cited in rossi, 1990) inarguably stated that the inner ear itself was a vibratory body and could therefore produce sound since it did not exist in a vacuum. however, instrumentation to measure this response had been slow to develop and his statements were largely ignored in favour of an exclusively sound-receiving cochlear, until the discovdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 carol a jardine ery of otoacoustic emissions (oae) by david kemp (1978). prior to this breakthrough, the ear was viewed as a passive transducer of sound and the idea that it could produce its own energy was inconceivable. kemp (1978) changed this perspective radically when he presented his discovery of a response measurable within the closed external acoustic meatus, more than 5 milliseconds after excitation. he termed these responses "cochlear echoes" (kemp, 1978, p. 1386) also referred to as "kemp echoes" (johnstone, patuzzi & yates, 1986, p. 147) and currently called " o t o a c o u s t i c e m i s s i o n s " (probst, antonellia & pieren, 1990, p.117; glattke & kujawa, 1991, p.29; and numerous others). upon his discovery, kemp (1978) hypothesized that there could be an augmentation of energy from the cochlea which would result in a secondary disturbance of the middle ear and eardrum. by doing so, he implied that some active processing was occurring in the cochlea causing a major upheaval in the last decade of audiology. since then, the traditional model proposed by von bekesy (1936 cited by zemlin, 1982) more than fifty years ago, has been expanded, manipulated, modified and at times rendered obsolete in the attempt to explain these oae. contemporary theories all aim to decipher the active, nonlinear, frequency selective processing of the cochlea. each model has its own unique orientation often based on examination of isolated aspects of cochlear physiology. unfortunately, none have been undeniably verified from research nor have these specialised viewpoints been amalgamated to form a cohesive model yet. indeed, it would seem that the process of audition is one of the most poorly understood physiological systems in medicine today. otoacoustic emissions using a signal averaging technique, a specially constructed acoustic probe and a broadband click stimulus, kemp (1978) recorded the well-documented, large initial middle ear response (0 5ms post stimulus) due to reflection of acoustical energy from the ear canal and tympanic membrane, as well as a much smaller response (5 62ms post stimulus) the hypothetical cochlear echo. responses of a 1.5 cc coupler with similar impedance characteristics to the human ear was compared to the human ear response as illustrated in figure 1. he found that both showed rapid deterioration of response to almost zero approximately 6ms post stimulus. however, the reappearance of a significant but much smaller response at about 10ms post stimulus was unique to the human ear and found consistently amongst normal hearing subjects, and subsequently known as evoked otoacoustic emissions (eoae) (kemp, 1978). such evidence led to speculations about the active function of the cochlea. distortion product otoacoustic emissions (dpoae) were subsequently discovered a year later (kemp, 1979). distortion products are usually generated by systems which are nonlinear (probst et al., 1990). stimulation of the acoustic system by two primary frequencies f ; and f2 will undergo intermodulation distortion and a response is generated at frequencies other than the primary ones but at specific mathematifigure 1. comparison between responses recorded in a coupler (a) and an adult human ear (b) showing the much smaller response at about 6ms poststimulus amplified in (c) (after glattke & kujawa, 1991, p. 31). cally-predictable frequencies ie. f, + f2, ft f2 and the most robust at 2ft f2 (probst et al., 1990; glattke & kujawa, 1991). nonlinearity of the cochlea is strongly suggested by this distortion of the signal. ι the third type of oae differs from the previous two in that it is generated in the absence of any evoking stimuli and known as spontaneous otoacoustic emissions (soae). they are measurable in approximately 40% of normal hearing people (bright & glattke, 1986 cited in glattke & kujawa, 1991) and are also thought to be representative of the active processes occurring in the cochlea. 1 the growth in technology together with the discovery of oae has thus led the way towards a more objective, in situ measurement of the non-linear, active role of the cochlea and refocussed the attention of biologists, physiologists and audiologists. cochlea as source of emissions there was initial speculation that this response could be due to protracted reflections within the middle ear structures (kemp, 1978) but glattke & kujawa (1991, p.29) refuted this concept because (a) the time delay of more than 6ms is sufficient time for a sound wave to travel more than 6 feet; (b) the duration of the response is much longer than either the duration of the stimulus of the time for the middle ear response to decay; and finally (c) the stimulating clicks produced well-defined, the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a new look at cochlear mechanics 61 frequency specific waveforms. johnson and elbering (1982) in examining the clinical utility of oae verified the cochlea as the source of these emissions by selecting two pathologies of known cochlear origin, that of aspirin toxicity and mumps. a moderate reversible salicylate-induced cochlear hearing loss was traced from pre-ingestion to two days following completion of the drug course. the initial bilateral emissions were reduced during the hearing loss but recovered fully after the drug was excreted suggesting to the researchers that the cochlea had recovered sufficiently. to ensure that these emissions were not an artifact of middle ear mechanisms, they tested a subject with complete anacusis in the right ear due to mumps. emissions could not be measured in the right ear, even in the presence of normal recordings in the left. these experiments led the authors to believe that the oae was of cochlear origin. this paper aims to briefly outline the basic anatomy of the cochlea and the classic theory of cochlear mechanics, before examining current models focusing on the macro-functioning of wave propagation, the cochlear cellular responses, and finally the biochemical activity within the ohc. historical perspective as early as 1857, helmholtz (cited in zemlin, 1982) proposed his resonance theory of hearing where he described the transverse fibres of the bm as being a bank of resonators. then von bekesy (1960 cited in clopton, 1986) was the first to formalise the one-dimensional travelling wave of the cochlea after extensive observations and experimentations spanning over half a decade (kim 1986), for which he was awarded the nobel prize in medicine and physiology in 1961 (zemlin, 1982). he postulated that spatial analysis of frequency information occurred as a result of the differential masses and stiffnesses along the length of the bm, each locus acting as a bank of resonators in a sense, but longitudinally coupled together by soft tissue (clopton, 1986). hydrodynamic forces within the fluids of the inner ear lead to displacement of the bm, and a shearing force between the rl and the tectorial membrane resulted in bending of the stereocilia; and electromechanical transduction (lim, 1986; nuttall, 1986). this view was supported by zwislocki (1946 cited in dancer 1992) and subsequent scrutinies and analyses have verified this very acceptable theory. yet, with the improvements in observation and model construction techniques, it appears that this macromodel of cochlear mechanics is now insufficient. the discrepancy between the broadly tuned data of the bm and the sharply sensitive auditory nerve was still confusing (johnstone et al., 1986). subsequently evans and wilson (1975 cited in clopton, 1986) suggested that there was a "second filter" to sharpen the frequency selectivity of the cochlea. the nature of this second filter has been open to much conjecture, with ideas ranging from microresonance of the bm to the gradient differences of the bm displacements (pickles, 1982). however, this concept of a second filter has generally been dismissed as being unnecesarry especially with the currently available experimental data (kim, 1986). indeed, lechner (1993) cautions against the use of passe models of bm motion because many models were based upon eclusively visual and often faulty observations. an example of such an occurrence is von bekesy's examination of longitudinal and radial fibres in the bm wherein he described that the incision of the bm resulted in openings in the shape of a cone (zemlin, 1982; lechner, 1993). one needs to bear in mind that several hours had elapsed between his observations and death of the tissue. voldrich (1978) repeating these experiments soon after cell death, found that the shape was in fact more radial, only becoming conical 24 hours following death the shape having far-reaching consequences in mathematical calculations. furthermore, nuttall (1986) highlights the poor instrumentation sensitivity used by von bekesy requiring intensity of at least 140dbspl (johnstone et al., 1986). therefore, a plethora of models was based on incorrect observations of a cadaver basilar membrane (von bekesy, 1960 cited in lechner, 1993) or following drainage of the scala tympani during preparation of an animal cochlea (dancer, 1992). active macromechanics in the cochlea the fundamentals of the travelling wave theory are generally still felt to hold true, that the cochlear is able to perform broad frequency analysis via differential displacement of the basilar membrane (neely & kim, 1986). nevertheless, the prevailing travelling wave theory has many limitations. it implies a cochlea which is a passive transducer to sound but if this was the case, it would be too highly damped to permit the sharp frequency selectivity to occur (clopton, 1986). the linear system model also implies that one could predict the response to a click from a previously measured frequency response to tone stimulation (eggermont, 1993, p. 177), but this does not hold true and indeed, geisler and sinex (1983 cited in eggermont, 1993) found that one could not even predict the response of a high intensity stimulus from that of a low intensity one. such inexplicable evidence, together with the discovery of oae and the ability of the cochlea to give rise to harmonic and intermodulation distortion and generate the dpoae, suggests that: the previously accepted anatomical and physiological processes were wholly inadequate to explain nonlinearity of the cochlea (eggermont, 1993) and that the inner ear was not as simple as first proposed by von bekesy in 1960 (kim, 1986, p.105). kemp tentatively suggested that at very low levels of stimulation, there may be hyperexcitation at certain frequencies resulting in a recoil of the bm at "localised impedance discontinuities" (1978, p. 1386) and the generation of the cochlea echo (1978). this postulation highlighted the possibility of the cochlea producing its own energy, sufficient to be transmitted retrograde through the middle ear and be generated within the ear canal. by constructing various cochlear models, several researchers have acknowledged the presence of 'some active element' to explain its fine tuning characteristics (neely and kim, 1986). contemporary theories expounding the mechanically active cochlea tend to agree with this axiom and imply that the travelling wave is amplified by initiating or supplementing existing motion in the bm. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 carol a jardine subsequent experimentation culminated in davis (1983) presenting a revolutionary new model of cochlear mechanics in which "an active process increases the vibration of the basilar membrane by energy provided somehow in the organ of corti" (p. 79). he termed this active process "the cochlear amplifier: (p.80) but subsequent researchers have referred to it as "negative impedance" and "negative damping" (neely & kim, 1986. p.1479), or "positive feedback" (de boer, 1983, p.571). effect of 1 2000 ht — cochlear amplifier 1 bekesy wave and envelope : / / » c / uj ο d t1 4 1 ^ t t ^ ^ / it j π / 2 phow s < v i . / \ \ x \ ii λ x vv « apex 1 f bote • k 16 1 12 10 8 6 4 mm i distance from stapes figure 2. the classic travelling wave and its envelope is presented together with the cochlear amplifier. note the frequency difference in amplitude peaks between the two (after davis, 1983, p. 82). the cochlear amplifier acts as a high-q (good frequency resolution) acoustic resonator according to davis (1983), such that it increases the sensitivity to lowthreshold stimuli, and sharp-tuning curves of a narrow segment of the bm. figure 2 illustrates a travelling wave with its envelope to which is added the effect of this cochlear amplifier. it is interesting to note that the tip of the cochlear amplifier does not correspond with the maximum displacement of the travelling wave. this theorem has since been corroborated by johnstone, patuzzi & yates (1986) who generated travelling wave envelopes with increasing intensity and found that at high intensities, the maximum displacement shifted half an octave lower than the stimulating frequency citing this as another example of the nonlinear property of the cochlea. the well-documented frequency differences between the noise exposure and its resultant temporary threshold shifts (about one-half octave) may be used as further substantiating evidence (davis, 1983, p.81). from a hydrodynamic perspective, kim,siegel & molnar (1979) on the other hand, proposed that the sharply tuned curves could occur as a result of negative damping along the crest of the travelling wave. by constructing a passive model of the cochlea, they observed a diffuse spread of energy through the fluid and assumed the total amount of energy entering the oval window was absorbed by the partition. in a contrasting active model, they constructed a negative damping partition just basal to the characteristic frequency, and hence energy in the form of eddy currents (according to neely & kim, 1986) is released from this point of the partition, resulting in an increase in energy equivalent to 40db which was sharply focussed. excessive gain at this point on the cochlear partition causes spontaneous oscillations of the hair cells thought to be the source of soae (neely & kim, 1986). neely & kim (1986) in an attempt to combine the mechanical and hydrodynamic properties of the cochlea into a single cohesive model, suggested that the cochlear amplifier not only occurs as a result of bm displacement but that the loci of maximum displacement coincides with the greatest pressure difference between the two fluid-filled compartments. they imply that this pressure differential acted as a selective tuner. von bekesy (1960 cited in zemlin, 1982) also reported observing an eddy current at the locus of maximum displacement during his experiments. active cellular responses although the mechanics of nonlinearity differ, there appears to be a consensus amongst researchers that "the bm vibration is very sharply tuned ... and is the predominant determinant in the major responses of the eighth nerve e.g., sensitivity, sharpness of tuning and many nonlinear functions" (johnstone et al., 1986, 148). despite these different theories, researchers believed that the ohc were responsible for this active process in the cochlea (clopton, 1986), and davis (1983) and bronwell (1990) postulated that it may be the energy source for the oae measurable in the external ear canal. noise, drugs (glattke & kujawa, 1991, p.30), anoxia and mechanical insults (davis, 1983, p.81) result in damage to ohc which then compromises the sensitivity (harrison and evans, 1979) and selectivity (sellick et al., 1982 cited in bronwell, 1990) of the cochlea, and can therefore be assumed to diminish or obliterate oae recordings a fact which has been verified through subsequent clinical trials. davis ( 1 9 8 3 ) a c k n o w l e d g e s that the active mocromechanism of the cochlea is very poorly 'understood and this has been the source of much debate. the motile properties of ohc are well accepted and it is popular to suggest that the lengthening or shortening of ohc could either move the rl and bm further] apart or closer together in order to deform the organ of corti and thereby produce the nonlinear response of the cochlea. caution against such simplistic explanations is voiced by de boer (1990). through a series of mathematical computations based upon the 'sandwich model' (refer figure 3), he proved that the motility of ohc would be unable to provide sufficient pressures to overcome the bm impedance and initiate motion. through observations and experimentations on the , cat cochlea, khanna, ulfendahl & flock (1990) assumed that the sharply tuned curves of the bm were sourced by the ohc. in subsequent research, khanna et al. (1990) were able to observe that the vibration amplitude and tuning characteristics for the ohc and the bm were different and concluded that they were independently controlled. the large vibration amplitude of the ohc suggested that the ohc-induced movement in the bm and bony shell of the cochlear and not vice versa previously^ supposed. such observations give the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a new look at cochlear mechanics 63 rl bm © t t rl bm j'prl © rl bm © figure 3. the "sandwich model" showing a filling of scala media (p3) with the organ of corti, and two wedges on either side, the scala vestibuli (p2) and tympani (pi) (after de boer, 1990, p.4). credibility to what was previously a revolutionary statement, even in this information era. moreover, they found that the vibration amplitudes at the level of the rl were maximal in the hensen cells, decreasing towards the ihc. the steepness of the slope, i.e. the tuning curve was sharpest in the third row of ohc, and shallowest in both the hensen's cells and ihc. this led the experimenters to conclude that the ohc drove the rl. the vibrations of the bm were small compared to that of the ohc and rl and it would seem that the primary source of energy lies in movement of the ohc carrying the rl with it. in a sense, de boer (1990) colludes with khanna et al. (1986) by refuting the possibility of bm movement. this evidence is in sharp contrast to that of the stereocilia bending due to the mechanical motions o f t h e b m . j for a closer examination of the role of rl mechanics, zwislocki (1986) constructed a model with nonlinear coupling of the rl and tectorial membrane. he noted that nonlinearities observed were similar to those of the auditory nerve firing patterns. at low intensity levels, there was little or no distortion, at slightly higher intensities there was splitting of the peaks, and at even higher intensities, there was a phase shift initially of the order of 90° and later 180°. he suggested that this nonlinearity could be due to the relative change in shearing movements between the tectorial membrane which is coupled to the ohc cilia and rl showing startlingly similar results after observing the rectified waveform of his construction. active micromechanical responses at a molecular level, the active mechanisms are less well understood but seem to have some basis in the contractile elements within the ohc. active lengthening and shortening of the ohc by stimulation of its actin filaments, have been demonstrated by several researchers in response to acoustic stimulation, drugs such as caffeine and potassium (slepecky, ulfendahl & flock, 1988; ulfendahl, flock & khanna, 1990) and electric currents (zenner, reuter, plinkert & gitter, 1990). most recently, it has been suggested that these changes can also be induced by mechanical activity, such as that observed in the movement of the cochlear partitions (kim, 1986; ulfendahl et al., 1990). kim (1986) therefore concluded, that it was important to examine the ohc whilst still within an intact organ of corti. by isolating the temporal bone from a guinea pig, they induced ohc shortening by applying caffeine. not surprisingly, the ohc did indeed shorten, and it appeared that the caffeine triggered the release of calcium ions (a substance known to trigger contractile activity in other muscles) (slepecky et al., 1988; zenner, 1990). moreover, the vibration amplitude of the organ of corti, and tuning of the mechanical response was sharper in the presence of caffeine. such conclusions led the experimenters to believe that the ohc were capable of influencing the mechanical response of the organ of corti and therefore played a much more active role in frequency selectivity, non-linearity and tuning frequency of the cochlea than previously supposed. zenner et al. (1990) reports that ohc activity can also be stimulated by electric currents. using a photodiode, they noted the longitudinal movements within the cylindrical cell body of the ohc, that there is shearing of the stereocilia via a lateral sliding of the cuticular plate and simultaneous movement of both the bm and rl in the same direction. the motion of these stereocilia will cause ohc coupling with the ihc via the tectorial membrane and is termed the "fast motile response" (zenner et al., 1990). it is unlikely that this response is due merely to the actin myosin interaction because of the short latency of the response according to ashmore and bronwell (1986 cited in slepecky et al., 1988). zenner et al. (1990) hypothesized that the displacement of the cuticular plate with the stereocilia may amplify the signal through closer coupling to the ihc via the tectorial membrane. the sharp focusing of these fast responses is thought to be responsible for the sharp tuning curves. an alternative explanation was suggested by kim (1986) in that these apically generated fast responses resulted in an oscillatory motion of the stereocilia. the idea of a bidirectional movement of the stereocilia has been revolutionary and illustrated through the examination of turtle hair cells (fettiplace, 1985 cited in nuttall, 1986). using this information, kim (1986) proposed that the forward transduction motion forms a feedback loop with the reverse motion. similar thoughts were expressed by clopton (1986) who felt that the voltage changes due to the stereocilia bending in one direction would result in greater excitation than in the other but does not suggest any specifics other than it may be sourced by the differential stiffness of the stereocilia depending upon direction of motion. however, other experimenters speculated that the asymmetrical arrangement of stereocilia at the apical ends of the ohc may account for the non-linearity of the cochlea (khanna et al., 1990, p23). a second, slower, motile mechanism of the ohc is also described wherein there is a slow depolarisation of ohc. shortening of the ohcs are observed, as is a reduction in the distance between the bm and rl. the origin of this response is thought to lie in the contraction of actin and myosin elements in the lateral wall of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 carol a jardine figure 4. schematic diagram of an outer hair cell illustrating proposed origins of the fast and slow motile responses and the apical areas rich in subcisternae (sc) (after kim, 1986, p. 110). the ohc (zenner, 1986) unlike the actin-independent fast motile responses. kim (1986) hypothesized that this slow motile response was responsible for the slow change in length of the ohc. figure 4 illustrates the anatomical origins of these fast and slow responses suggested by kim (1986). there is much uncertainty over whether the fast, motile responses or the slower secondary ones give rise to the selectivity, nonlinearity and sensitivity of the cochlea although much research has been focused on the existence of efferent feedback loops. efferent fibres from the oliviocochlear bundle are thought to control the fast, amplifying and selective action of the cochlea. efferent control a source of nonlinearity contralateral masking is able to suppress ipsilateral dpoae (puel, rebillard & pujol, 1990), and soae (kujawa & glattke, 1989). siegel & kim (1982) found that stimulation of the contralateral fibres arising from the oliviocochlear bundle (which synapse mainly with the ohc) could alter the strength of the distortion products suggesting ohc involvement in the generation of otoacoustic emissions. already in 1962 (fex cited in warren iii & lieberman, 1989) reported that the cochlear efferent system, arising from the oliviocochlear nucleus (ocn) could inhibit activity in the auditory afferent fibres. a schematic diagram of the anatomical •pathway of the efferent fibres can be found in figure 5. however, warren iii et al. (1989, p.98) report that it had not been conclusively proven that this efferent system arose from the ocn only and they highlight three methods where contralateral sound can influence responses to the ipsilateral stimulus: 1. mechanical propagation of the acoustic stimulus via bone conduction to the contralateral cochlea; figure 5. the cochlear efferent system originating from the oliviocochlear nucleus in the superior olivary complex. 2. some neural feedback system of which there could be three possibilities: (a) via the middle ear reflex muscles (stapedius and tensor tympani); (b) the oliviocochlear efferent system and (c) autonomic efferents to blood vessels within the spiral lamina: 3. there may be humoral agents released when the ear is acoustically stimulated and which travel via the circulatory system to the opposite side. the idea of accoustic crosstalk was eliminated since the intensity of the sound was always less than the level required for contralateral excitation. likewise, the likelihood of blood circulating agents was also discounted because of the time necessary to travel to the opposite cochlea. the three remaining neural feedback systems appear to be the most feasible explanation. both warren iii et al. (1989) and puel et al. (1990) in theirjstudies severed the stapedius and tensor tympani muscles to ensure that the middle ear muscles did not participate in this phenomenon. finally, through a process of elimination warren iii et al. (1989) state that the oliviocochlear system is the most likely originator of the efferent feedback loop since they argue unconvincingly that when there is removal of the cervical synpathetic system, there is a decrease in the n1 action potential (hultzcrantz et al., 1982 cited in warren iii et al., 1989) suggesting to them that the autonomic nervous system was in fact used to enhance auditory sensitivity an idea first suggested by beickert et al., 1956 (cited in zemlin, 1982) although to date, there is still no empirical evidence. warren iii et al. (ibid) therefore attributed this feedback process to the inhibitory cochlear efferents innervating the ohc and originating from the ocn in the central auditory pathway. they further postulate that the medial ocn was responsible for this action because they comprise large, myelinated nerves whereas the lateral ocn consists of largely small, unmyelinated neurones from experiments comparing myelinated and unmyelinated responses to electrical excitation (gifford & guinam, 1987). the idea of a closed feedback loop system and cochlear the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a new look at cochlear mechanics efferents to modulate ohc motility is also proposed by puel et al., (1990), kim (1986) and bronwell (1990). animal observations by lieberman (1988 cited in warren hi et al., 1989 and in froehlich, collet, valatz & morgon, 1993) show that auditory afferent stimuli can excite the efferent fibres arising from the medial ocn i.e. large, unmyelinated efferents leading to the ohc (warr et al., 1986 cited in dannhof & bruns, 1993). moreover, these medial ocn efferent fibres could inhibit activity in the contralateral auditory efferent fibres (lieberman 1988 cited in warren iii et al., 1989). johnstone et al. (1986) suggested that these efferents were used to return the stereocilia to its normal position after being bent in an excitatory manner by the movement of the bm (p. 151). in another paper with rajan (1983), he also noted the protective effects of stimulating the efferent system during tts and cited this as evidence of their role in the feedback loop. the high frequency selectivity of the bat has always sparked interest and its efferent innervation was of particular interest to xie, henson, bishop & henson (1993). they identified a greater number of ohc terminals in areas known to be associated with sharp frequency tuning and selectivity in the bat. in study of mammals such as the cat (lieberman & brown, 1986) and the guinea pig (hashimoto & kimura, 1987), the trend is towards a greater density of efferent terminals in the basal and middle regions with less in the apical regions. a comparison of vibration responses of the third and fourth turn in the guinea pig cochlea show that there is a sharper tuning in the more basal ends (khanna et al., 1990) an observation confirmed by using in vivo measurements of the cat cochlea (lieberman, 1982 cited in neely & kim, 1986). a relationship between greater frequency sensitivity and efferent terminal density is therefore implied by these observations (xie et al., 1993). not only is there a regional variation in efferent innervation, but also differences across the rows of ohc noted by several researchers (fex et al. 1982; simmons et al. 1990; fex & altschuler 1984). initially, the density of efferent fibres along the first row of ohc was thought to be a consequence of developmental patterns but it has been suggested that this is not so. the population density may be relatejd to amount of synaptic activity and possibly areas of sharp tuning (xie et al., 1993). however, this in sharp contrast to findings by khanna et al. (1990) who found that the vibration amplitude and frequency response was sharpest in the third row of ohc. however, xie et al. (1993) acknowledges that the role of efferent terminals are still unclear and subject to much conjecture. a lone voice amongst this excitement about efferent feedback loops is that of dolan & nuttall (1989) who suggested that there may be no feedback loop or efferent control system since they observed changes in ihc potentials following contralateral stimulation to be constant in amplitude and instantaneous in activation. they remark that such a feedback system would not be able to be activated within such a short latency and with such constant magnitude. cochlear biochemistry i the bending of the cilia is thought to generate an electric current (depolarisation) along their apical ends 65 which controls the release of neurotransmitter vesicles at the base of the hair cell (clopton, 1986) and supposedly initiates firing of the auditory nerve (nuttall, 1986). this characteristic of the ohc in which they are capable of bending in both directions with force distinguishes it from skeletal muscle in which contractions can only be directed in a single direction, whilst the relaxation phase is passive and does not exert force (bronwell, 1990). lim (1986) is more specific, and suggests that the stereocilia bending in the excitatory direction only will result in the initiation of neural impulses. nevertheless, depolarisation of drug-induced contractions result in an increase in intracellular ca2 +. nuttall (1986) speaks about the "gating of ions" (p.29), or a physical channel through the cell membrane permitting the passage of ions through. zenner (1986) demonstrated that by bathing the hair cells in variable ionic concentrated fluid could result in motile responses of the hair cells therefore suggesting as nuttall (1986) does, that extracellular ions are responsible for motility of the cilia. ikeda & takasaka (1993) more specifically reported that the i n c r e a s e in i n t r a c e l l u l a r c a 2 + was due to transmembrane influx of calsium from the surrounding endolymph and release from the apical cisterns and hensen's cells. contradictory evidence is presented by slepecky et al. (1988) in a study of the effects of tetracaine, a local anaesthetic when they found that it inhibited potassium and calcium-induced contractions in muscle but it did not affect contractions of the ohc when applied extracellularly. this led them to the believe that the increase in cellular calcium was of intracellular origin and did not arise from an influx through the permeable cell membrane. kim (1986) postulated that this slow response was mediated in the area between the cuticular plate and the ohc nucleus an area characterised by a large number of these subsurface cisternae. the finding of gamma a m i n o b u y t y r i c acid (eybauling et al., 1988 cited in dannhof et al., 1993) and acetylcholine receptors (zenner et al., 1990) at the synapses of the ohc, suggested that efferent nerves could stimulate release of inositol-trisphosphate which would control the release and uptake of calcium ions into the cytoplasm and modulate ohc contractions. recent discovery of other neurotransmitters such as choline acetyltransferase and glutamatee decarboxylase present in all ohc efferents and along the entire cochlea (dannhof et al., 1993) suggested that they perform a similar function. it is unlikely that the fast motile responses required the mediation of intracellular ca 2 + (zenner et al., 1987 cited in ikeda & takasaka, 1993) because of its relative independence of adenosin triphosphate (atp) involvement (kachar et al., 1986 cited in slepecky et al., 1988). de boer (1990) speculates that mechanical energy within the scala media or tectorial membrane could be released into the organ of corti, or that the energy source could be electrical or even chemical, and idea that appeals to neely and kim (1986) as well. they guessed that the source of this e n e r g y was electrochemically obtained from the surrounding endolymph. atp is necessary in all biological systems as an energy source necessary for the maintenance of the system. an active cochlea implies that energy needs to be added to the system and schacht (1986) reasons die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 carol a jardine that there is no evidence that the cochlea differs from any other system in the body, and that atp is formed through energy metabolism procedure called oxidative phosphorylation. atp is a phosphate compound whose molecular bonds are rich in energy which can be released when required (mcgilvery & goldstein, 1979). bronwell (1990) strongly disagrees with schacht (1986) stating that "the movements result from direct conversion of electrical potential energy to mechanical energy come from experiments that demonstrate movements even after cellular stores of atp are depleted" (p.84). by so doing, he is specifically stating that the physiology of the cochlea is very different to any other body system by not requiring atp. he states that atp is produced in stria vascularis and used to drive peripheral cells and not ohc specifically. davis (year unknown, cited by schacht, 1986) was the first to present his "battery theory" where he cites stria vascularis as being the source of this energy and for this reason schacht (1986) surmises that the cochlea has an unusually low metabolic rate in comparison to other sensory systems because of the contribution of stria vascularis although the exact relationship has yet to be elucidated. four years later, bronwell suggested that the stria vascularis is the source of a silent current of potential energy (1990) and it is this current that stimulates the motility of the ohc. ohc are therefore indirectly fueled by atp via the stria vascularis. return to the passive model lechner (1993).correctly states that the activity of the intact bm is still unclear. dancer (1992), from a review of the literature also suggests the re-examination of the bm. if the cochlea is as active as suggested by the previous research, clopton (1986) argues that such an undamped system would be highly unstable and would have a long release time with oscillations continuing for several seconds. this instability is acknowledged by bronwell (1990) who rationalises that it is these very oscillations which will trigger soae. by comparing the latency between the cochlear microphonic and the input stimulus obtained via direct measurements and those calculated from the zwislocki travelling wave theory, dancer (ibid) was able to conclude that measurements were significantly smaller than those calculated, as illustrated in figure 6. he concluded that the bm was stimulated as a whole immediately and that the bm acted as a helmholtz resonator at least at frequencies below the characteristic frequency, a theory subscribed to more than a century before. however, he is sceptical about the use of the onedimensional longwave model proposed by zwislocki (1948 cited in zemlin, 1982) because the expectations of a long latency required by this model are not met by the short experimental measurements. they concluded , that this bank of resonators was a passive phenomenon until the point of characteristic frequency of the bm, where ohc are thought to contribute towards some actiye p r o c e s s i n g s u p e r i m p o s e d upon a passive hydromechanical phenomenon. clopton (1986) also subscribes to this view, in which the passive role of bm resonance is coupled with the active but independent resonation of the tectorial membrane and hair cells. lizard hair cells were examined cochlear distance, mm figure 6. time taken for a wave to traverse the length of a guinea pig cochlea. theoretically calculated times ( ) are contrasted with actual latencies ( • • • • ) (after dancer, 1992, p. 305). by peake & ling (1980) and they found that the bm vibrated as a whole in response to acoustic stimuli and no travelling wave was generated. using a newly developed piezopolymer transducer to measure bm displacement, lechner (1993) constructed a hydromechanical model of the ear and concluded from his observations that the longitudinal stiffness of the bm could provide differential response characteristics, seemingly providing more experimental evidence that perhaps these traditional theories were not to be discarded so quickly. kolston, viergever, de boer & diependaal (1989) criticise the assumption of active elements in the cochlea since it was based on observed broad mechanical tuning together with the fine frequency selectivity of the neural responses. in comprehensive cochlea model based on an anatomical evidence, kolston et al. (1989) suggest that the bm is divided into an arcuate and pectinate zone (refer figure 7). the motion of the arcuate zone was influenced by the subtectorial j space | organ of corti arcuate zone j pectinate zone bm figure 7. schematic diagram showing the division of the basilar membrane (bm) into an arcuate and pectinate zone (after kolston et al., 1989, p. 134). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a new look at cochlear mechanics 67 organ of corti and tectorial membranes. they postulated that the supporting cells in the pectinate zone acted as feedback mechanisms to the ohc to enable them to control their impedance and consequently indirectly controlling bm motion. this theory is interesting in that it is able to suggest a function for the supporting cells as well which up to now has been speculated to be purely supportive and nutritive (engstrom, 1982). this theory is also able to explain the contrast between broad tuning measured in the cochlea and the sharpness of the acoustic nerve response (johnstone et al., 1986). the authors argue that the probes utilised measured average bm response, largely the response of the pectinate zone. their conclusion is that sharp frequency selectivity of the cochlea can exist without the use of active elements, if the effect of the arcuate zone were to be taken into account. data from khanna & leonard (1982 cited in neely & kim, 1986) speculate that there is a linear relationship between bm displacement and excitation of hair cells at levels near threshold. de boer (1983) then extended this idea by suggesting that nonlinearity only becomes evident at high intensities. this idea correlates well with the hypothesis that only excessive gain of the amplifier would generate soae (neely & kim, 1986). several theories subscribe to the coexistence of a passive and active element in the cochlea, but differ in their division of the roles be it along the threshold of frequency continuum. and so ... despite this wealth of data about the ohc, what about the inner hair cells (ihc). spoendlin (1969 cited in engstrom et al., 1982) reports that 90 95% of the afferent nerve fibres innervate the ihc. moreover, most of the auditory information carried by the auditory nerve arise from the ihc (pfingst, 1986). the purpose of the ihc seems to be to detect the stimulus and mimic the characteristics of the acoustic nerve (de boer, 1983; nuttall, 1986). how the jstereocilia of these cells are displaced is still questionable although there are postulations that it may be through the viscous forces of the surrounding fluids (nuttall, 1986) or influence from the active mechanical ohc. brown & nuttall (1984 cited in nuttall, 1986) found that the ohc has sufficient active energy to set the inner hair cell cilia in motion. the exact nature of the coupling between ohc and ihc is still one of the most perplexing question in auditory physiology since it has been generally accepted that there is no direct mechanical coupling via the tectorial membrane. nevertheless, lim (1986) in his detailed review of the microanatomy of the cochlea reports on the existence of ihc imprints on the basal surface of the tectorial membrane. he acknowledges that these are not as distinct and consistent in appearance as those of the ohc but at the same time states that the possibility of ihc (especially those at the apical end) coupling to the tectorial membrane albeit loose should not be ignored. such evidence suggests that there could be mechanical coupling of ohc responses to the ihc. by anatomically contrasting the ihc and ohc, a number of differences between these two sensory structures can be noted, most markedly the innervation system (greater number of afferents ihc), tectorial membrane coupling and location along the bm (lim, 1986, p. 142) . ihc tends to be located on the immobile portion of the bm whilst ohc are found on the lateral, more mobile portions. this suggests that the ohc could be responsible for the active mechanics of the cochlea whilst the ihc acts as the passive transducer. the role of the supporting cells in auditory perception has also been questioned although evidence is still scanty. traditionally, they have been considered to provide a supporting framework, form a boundary between the endolymph and perilymph, formation of the tectorial membrane and phagocytosis of cell debris (engstrom et al., 1982; oesterle & dallos, 1989). oesterle & dallos (1986) found that both alternating and direct current were compared and the amplitude of the currents were found to be dependent upon the cell location. the closer the cell was situated to the ihc, the larger the direct current component. they postulated that their role could be in the regulation of neurotransmitter concentration in the intercellular spaces (p.231). in a subsequent paper on the same subject, the experimenters concluded that the potentials measured in the supporting cells arise from the electric currents generated by the hair cells and also show a reflection of the tuned responses of the hair cells. conclusions research in the area of cochlear micromechanics is still ongoing although much information has been gained through the discovery of otoacoustic emissions. recording of spontaneous and evoked emissions revived the concept that the cochlea could be active, whilst the discovery of distortion product emissions confirmed the presence of nonlinearity in the cochlea. the third problem facing biophysicists was the high frequency selectivity of the ihc and viii nerve when compared to the broad tuning curves of the bm. subsequent research has focussed mainly on the resolution of these three aspects of cochlear physiology. there is a wealth of fragmentary experimental data examining isolated cochlear behaviour under certain conditions, but the assimilation of this data into a cohesive cochlear model is still elusive. a major problem lies in the construction of a mechanically active cochlear model which is intrinsically unstable hampering investigators (de boer, 1983, p.572) as well as the limited in vivo measurements possibly causing the seemingly contradictory data. regardless of these difficulties, there seems to be increasing concensus over several issues: (a)that non-linearity exists at several levels the basilar m e m b r a n e , the m e c h a n i c a l t r a n s d u c t i o n into potentials and between the hair cell potentials themselves (eggormont, 1993); (b)that there is an active process occurring within the cochlear (davis, 1983) primarily mediated by the outer hair cells (kim, 1986); (c) that the origin of the emissions are the electromotility of the ohc (bronwell, 1990) as observed by the obliteration of these responses following noise-exposure, salicylate ingestion and other insults; (d)that the efferent feedback loop (xie et al., 1993) and in particular the neurotransmitters (zanner et al., die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 carol a jardine 1990) play a critical role in this. such observations would explain the sharp tuning curves of the cochlea and the presence of otoacoustic emissions and the pivotal role the ohc plays. nevertheless, some authors have cautioned against the quick dismissal of the traditionally held theories of hearing and suggest the presence of some crucial passive response of the cochlea occurring concomitantly (dancer, 1992). there is an overwhelming mass of experimental data on cochlear mechanics and each publication brings with it a new dimension. this paper serves merely to highlight the current trends, but this arena is eternally in a state of flux. undoubtedly, the cochlea is truly a sensitive, selective and secretive instrument! references bronwell, w.e. (1990). outer hair cell electromotility and otoaustic emissions, ear and hearing, 11, 2, 82-92. carlisle, l. (1986). anatomy and pathology of the inner ear. seminars in hearing, 7,1, 1-14. clopton, b.m. (1986). micromechanics of the cochlea. seminars in hearing, 7,1, 15-26. dannhof, b.j. & bruns, v. (1993). the innervation of the organ of corti in the rat. hearing research, 66, 8-22. dancer, a. (1992). experimental look at cochlear mechanics. audiology, 31, 301-312. davis, h. (1983). an active process in cochlear mechanics. hearing research, 9, 79-90. de boer, e. (1983). no sharpening? a challenge for cochlear mechanics. journal of acoustical society of america, 73, 2, 567-573. de boer, e. (1990). wave propagation, activity and frequency selectivity in the cochlea. in f. grandori, g. cianfrone, d.t. kemp (eds): cochlear mechanisms and otoacoustic emissions, advances in audiology. basel, karger, 7, 1-12. dolan, d.f. and nuttall, a. (1989). inner hair cell response to tonal stimulation in the presence of broadband noise. journal of acoustical society of america, 86, 3, 1007-1012. eggermont, j.j. (1993). wiener and volterra analyses applied to the auditory system. hearing research. 66, 177-201. engstrom, h. and engstrom, b. (1982). hearing: some notes on structure and function, topholm and westermann, denmark. fex, j., altschuler, r.a., wenthold, r.j. & parakkal, m.h. (1982). aspartate aminotransferase immunoreactivity in cochlear of guinea pig. hearing research, 7, 149-160. fex, j. & altschuler, r.a. (1984). glutamic acid decarboxylase immunoreactivity of olivocochlear neurones in the organ of corti of guinea pig and rat. hearing research, 15, 123131. froelich, p., collet, l., valatz, j.l. & morgon, a. (1993). sleep and active cochlear micromechanical properties in human subjects. hearing research, 66, 1-7. gifford, m.l. & guinam, j.j. (1987). effects of electrical stimulation of medial oliviocochlear neurons on ipsilateral and contralateral cochlear responses. hearing research, 29, 179-194. glattke, t.j. & kujawa, s.g. (1991). otoacoustic emissions. american journal of audiology, 29-40. grandori, f., cianfrone, g. & kemp, d.t. (1990). preface to cochlear mechanisms and otoacoustic emissions. advances in audiology. basel, karger vii-viii. harisson, r.v. & evans, e.f. (979). cochlear fibre responses in guinea pigs with well defined cochlear lesions. scandinavian audiology suppl, 9, 83-92. hashimoto, s. & kimura, r.s. (1987). computer-aided three dimensional reconstruction and morphometry of the outer hair cells of the guinea pig cochlea. acta otolaryngologica, 103, 64-74. hultzcrantz, e., nuttall, .a.l., brown, m.c. & lawrence, m. (1982). the effect of cervical sympathectomy on cochlear physiology, acta otolaryngologica, 94, 439-444. ikeda, k. & takasaka, t. (1993). confocal laser microscopical images of calcium distribution and intracellular organelles in the outer hair cell isolated from the guinea pig cochlea. hearing research, 66, 169-176. johnsen, n.j. & elberling, c. (1982). evoked acoustic emissions from the human ear: i. equipment and response parameters. scandinavian audiology, 11, 3-12. johnstone, b.m., patuzzi, r. & yates, g.k. (1986). basilar membrane measurements and the travelling wave. hearing research, 22, 147-153. kemp, d.t. (1978). stimulated acoustic emissions from within the human auditory system. journal of the acoustical society of america, 64, 5, 1386-1391. kemp, d.t. (1979). evidence of mechanical nonlinearity and frequency selective wave amplification in the cochlea. archives of otorhinolaryngology, 244, 37-45. khanna, s.m. & leonard, d.g.b. (1986). measurement of basilar membrane vibrations and evaluation of cochlear condition. hearing research, 23, 55-70. khanna, s.m., ulfendahl, m. & flock, a. (1990). cellular mechanical responses in the cochlea. in f. grandori, g. cianfrone, d.t. kemp (eds): cochlear mechanisms and otoacoustic emissions. advances in audiology. basel, karger, 7, 13-26. kim, d.o., siegel, j.h. & molnar, c.e. (1979). cochlear nonlinear phenomena in two-tone response. scandinavian audiology suppl, 9, 63-81. kolston, p.j., viergever, m.a., de boer, e. & diependaal, r.j. (1989). realistic mechanical tuning in the micromechanical cochlear model. journal of acoustical society of america, 81, 1, 133-139. kujawa, s.g. & glattke, t.j. (1989, october). influence of contralateral acoustic stimulation on spontaneous otoacoustic emissions. asha, 31, 123. lechner, t.p. (1993). a hydromechanical model of the cochlea with nonlinear feedback using pvf2 bending transducers. hearing research, 66, 202-212. lieberman, m.c. & brown, m.c. (1986). physiology and anatomy of single oliviocochlear neurons in the cat, hearing research, 24, 17-36. lim, d.j. (1986). functional structure of the organ of corti: a review. hearing research, 22, 117-146. mcgilvery, r.w. & goldstein, r. (1979). biochemistry: a functional approach, 2nd edition, wb saunders co., japan. neely, s.t. &kim, d.o. (1986). a model for active elements in cochlear biomechanics, journal of acoustical society of america, 79, 5, 1472-1480. nuttall, a.l. (1986). transduction and frequency tuning in hair cells. seminars in hearing, 7, 1, 27-44. oesterle, e. & dallos, p. (1986). intracellular recordings from supporting cells in the organ of corti. hearing research, 22, 229-232. j oesterle, e. & dallos, p. (1989). intracellular recordings from supporting cells in the guinea-pig cochlea: ac potentials, journal of acoustical society of america, 86, 3, 1013-1032. peake, w.t. & ling, a. (1980). basilar membrane motion in the alligator lizard: its relation to tonotopic organisation and frequency selectivity, journal of acoustical society, of america, 67, 1736-45. ' pfingst, b.e. (1986). encoding of frequency and level information in the auditory nerve. seminars in hearing, 7, 1, 45-64. ' pickles, j.o. (1982). an introduction to the physiology of hearing, academic press, london. probst, r., antonellia, c. & pieren, c. (1990). methods and preliminary results of measurements of distortion product otoacoustic emissions in normal and pathological ears. in f. grandori, g. cianfrone & d.t. kemp (eds): cochlear mechanisms and otoacoustic emissions, advances in audiology. basel, karger, 7, 117-125. puel, j.l., rebillard, g. & pufol, r. (1990), active mechanisms and cochlear efferents. in f. grandori, g. cianfrone & d.t. kemp (eds): cochlear mechanisms and otoacoustic emissions, advances in audiology. karger, 7, 156-163. rajan, g. (1990). crossed cochlear influences, on monaural temporary threshold shifts, hearing research, 12, 185-197. rossi, g. (1990). intracochlear mechanisms involved in the generation of delayed evoked otoacoustic emissions. in f. grandori, g. cianfrone & d.t. kemp (eds): cochlear mechanisms and otoacoustic emissions. advances in the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a new look at cochlear mechanics 69 audiology. basel, karger, 7, 180-187. schacht, j. (1986). biochemistry of cochlear function and pathology. seminars in hearing, 7, 1, 101-115. siegel, j.h. & kim, d.g. (1982). efferent neural control of cochlear mechanics? olivocochlear bundle stimulation affects cochlear biomechanical nonlinearity. hearing research, 6, 171-182. simmons, d.d., manson-gieseke, l., hendrix, t.w. & mccarter, s. (1990). reconstructions of efferent fibres in the postnatal hamster cochlea, hearing research, 15, 113122. slepecky, n., ulfendahl, m. & flock, a. (1988). effects of caffeine and tetracain on outer hair cell shortening suggest intracellular calcium involvement. hearing research, 32, 11-22. ulfendahl, m., flock, a. & khanna, s.m. (1990). cochlear mocromechanics from isolated cells to the intact hearing organ. in f. grandori, g. cianfrone & d.t. kemp (eds): cochlear mechanisms and otoacoustic emissions. advances in audiology. basel, karger, 7, 27-34. voldrich, l. (1978). mechanical properties of basilar membrane. acta otolaryngologica, 86, 331-335. warren iii, e.h. & lieberman, m.c. (1989). effects of contralateral sound on auditory nerve responses: i. contributions of cochlear efferents. hearing research, 37, 89-104. xie, d.h., henson, m.m., bishop, a.l. & henson, o.w. (1993). efferent terminals in the cochlea of the mustached bat: quantitative data. hearing research, 66, 81-90. zemlin, w.r. (1982). speech and hearing science anatomy and physiology, 2nd edition, prentice-hall inc, new jersey. zenner, h.p., zimmerman, u. & schmitt, u. (1985). reversible contraction of isolated mammalian cochlear hair cells, hearing research, 18, 127-133. zenner, h.p. (1986). motile responses in outer hair cells, hearing research, 22, 83-90. zenner, h.p., reuter, g., plinkert, p.k. & gitter, a.h. (1990). fast and slow motility of outer hair cells in vitro and in situ. in f. grandori, g. cianfrone & d.t. kemp (eds): cochlear mechanisms and otoacoustic emissions. advances in audiology. basel, karger, 7, 35-41. zwislocki, j.j. (1986). are nonlinearities observed in firing rates of auditory nerve afferents reflections of a nonlinear coupling between the tectorial membrane and the organ of corti?. hearing research, 22, 217-221. address correspondence to: c jardine, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, ρ ο wits, 2050. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 acoustimed p.o. box 9 9 8 8 hearing services joh—urg offices in melville and johannesburg tel: 337-2977 fax: 337-5579 tinnitus maskers tinnitus is usually experienced as a ringing, hissing or buzzing sound in the ear or in the head. it is commonly associated with a mild hearing loss in the high frequencies. according to the american tinnitus association almost 5% of the american population suffers from tinnitus in its severe form and millions more are afflicted to a lesser degree. we have no reason to believe that the situation is any different in south africa. surveys have shown that the most effective source of relief is a tinnitus masker. acoustimed's tinnitus masker is an in-the-canal device which uses a specially designed amplifier module capable of amplifying up to 20000hz. for high frequency tinnitus the masker can be adjusted to provide as much as 70db spl of masking noise at frequencies above 6000hz. our experience with tinnitus masking has shown that a high frequency masker can provide effective masking with least awareness of the masking noise. in exceptional cases the masking noise is inaudible. the presence of a masker interferes with the acoustic characteristics of the ear canal causing, in effect, a mild high frequency hearing loss. all maskers must, therefore, provide some acoustic gain in the region of 3000hz even if the patient's hearing is completely normal. where necessary the masker will additionally compensate tor the hearing loss which is typically associated with tinnitus. because each masker is custom-built it is necessary to determine the nature of the tinnitus and obtain a reasonable prognosis prior to manufacture. for this purpose we use a number of instruments including a computer program which simulates multi-frequency tinnitus. for more information please telephone 337-2977 or 726-2430. registered hearing aid acousticians: d. a. smith d. christodoulides l. williams the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 199 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 53 die verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van leergestremdes idillette qosthuizen, brenda louw en isabel uys departement kommunikasiepatologie, universiteit van pretoria opsomming die ondersoek het dit ten doel gehad om die verband tussen gesproke taal, geskrewe taal en fonologiese prosessering te bepaal. die empiriese opname is deur middel van 'n beskikbaarheidseleksie op nege afrikaanssprekende taalleergestremde leerlinge in graad-twee uitgevoer. ooreenkomstig voorafbepaalde doelstellings is gesproke taalparameters, geskrewe taalparameters en fonologiese prosesseringsparameters geidentifiseer. die proefpersone se peil van funksionering op die onderskeie parameters, sowel as die verwantskap tussen die parameters is kwalitatief en kwantitatief ontleed en beskryf. die resultate van hierdie empiriese opname toon dat betekenisvolle korrelasies tussen gesproke taal, geskrewe taal en fonologiese prosesseringsvaardighede by die betrokke taalleergestremde proefpersone voorgekom het. abstract the aim of this study was to explore the interrelationship between spoken language, written language and phonological processing. the empirical study was administered on nine afrikaans-speaking, language-impaired children in grade two by means of accidental sampling. conformable to predetermined objectives, spoken language parameters, written language parameters, and phonological processing parameters were identified. the subjects' level of functioning with regard to each parameter, and the interrelationship between these parameters were qualitatively and quantitatively analysed and described. the results of this study indicated significant correlations between spoken language, written language and phonological processing abilities of the selected language impaired subjects. sleutelwoorde: taalleergestremdheid, gesproke taal· i geletterdheid, geletterdheidsontwikkeling en voorlopers tot geletterdheid is tans van die mees kontensieuse onderwerpe binne die vakgebied van spraak-taalpatologie. 'n herorientasie in navorsers en beroepslui se siening van geletterdheid het gedurende)die afgelope eeu en veral gedurende die afgelope dekade plaasgevind. voor 1970 is leesprobleme as 'n uitval in visueel-perseptuele prosesse beskou. johnson en myklebust (1967) het egter die grondslag gele vir die perspektief dat leesprobleme, en gevolglik ook skryfprobleme, 'n taalgebaseerde uitval is en nie noodwendig 'n refleksie van kognitiewe of visueelperseptuele beperkinge nie (kamhi & catts, 1991). hierdie verandering in siening van navorsers en beroepslui bring mee dat taal as vertrekpunt vir die ontwikkeling van gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens beskou word. die verandering stel opnuut uitdagende en opwindende eise aan die spraak-taalterapeut. juis vanwee die spraak-taalterapeut se besondere kennis van taalontwikkeling en taalprobleme, speel lede van hierdie beroep η kardinale rol in diagnose en intervensie van die kind met gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme. die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak-taal-gehoor (savstg) (1990: 2) ondersteun die standpunt dat die spraaktaalterapeut 'n belangrike rol speel tydens diagnose en intervensie van beide gesproke taalprobleme en geskrewe geskrewe taal, fonologiese prosessering. taalprobleme. dit blyk dat vanwee die feit dat die spraak-taalterapeut 'n onmiskenbare rol by beide gesproke en geskrewe taal speel, dit noodsaaklik geword het om weg te beweeg van 'n diskrete-puntbenadering waar gesproke taal alleen aangespreek word, 'n breer en dus holistiese benadering word aanbeveel waar die kind in totaliteit gesien word en beide gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme as uitvloeisel van 'n onderliggende taalprobleem behandel word. die noue interaksie tussen gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme bestaan en die feit dat die spraak-taalterapeut gevolglik albei di£ komponente tydens diagnose en intervensie by die leergestremde kind behoort aan te spreek, impliseer dat groter eise en hoer vaardigheidsvlakke aan beroepslui gestel word. dit is van kardinale belang dat navorsing oor taalprobleme, en die gevolg van taalprobleme, voortgesit moet word sodat resente bevindings by die bestaande databasis gevoeg kan word (klecan-aker & kelty, 1990). as gevolg van ontwikkeling in die vakgebied, ontstaan daar voortdurend nuwe vraagstukke wat navorsing vereis. nuwe kennis moet voortdurend by bestaande kennis gevoeg word, sodat die spraak-taalterapeut 'n teoretiese basis kan formuleer wat as grondslag kan dien vir effektiewe diagnosering en intervensie by die taalleergestremde kind. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 idilette oosthuizen, brenda louw en isabel uys in die lig van resente navorsingsbevindinge word die spraak-taalterapeut gekonfronteer met vraagstukke soos wat die spesifieke aard van die verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme werklik is (stackhouse, 1990). dit is vir verantwoordbare beroepsoptrede noodsaaklik dat die spraak-taalterapeut kennis moet dra van hierdie vraagstukke. vanwee die.noue verwantskap tussen gesproke taalfunksionering en geskrewe taalfunksionering kan gesproke taal en geskrewe taal nie as sodanig in isolasie benader word nie. in die verlede is die rol van die spraak-taalterapeut afgebaken en beperk tot evaluering, diagnose en intervensie van gesproke taalprobleme. hierdie fragmentariese beskouing het tot gevolg gehad dat daar tans 'n leemte in kennis oor die spesifieke verwantskap tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal bestaan. bo en behalwe dat gesproke taal en geskrewe taal as interafhanklike vaardighede beskou word, word daar tans gespekuleer oor die invloed van fonologiese prosessering op beide hierdie vaardighede. daar word in resente publikasies (stackhouse, 1990; kamhi & catts, 1991) gehipotetiseer dat gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme moontlik die manifestasie van dieselfde onderliggende fonologiese prosesseringsprobleem is. stackhouse (1990) beskryf pertinent die moontlikheid van spraak-, leesen spelprobleme as verskillende manifestasies van dieselfde onderliggende fonologiese prosesseringsprobleem. waar kinders met gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme presenteer, ontstaan die vraag of 'n onderliggende fonologiese prosesseringsprobleem kan bydra, of selfs die primere oorsaak van beide gesproke taalprobleme en geskrewe taalprobleme, kan wees. indien laasgenoemde wel die geval is, impliseer dit dat 'n verbetering in fonologiese prosesseringsvermoens 'n verbetering in beide die gesproke taalvaardigheid en geskrewe taalvaardigheid teweeg sou kon bring. die behoefte om meer inligting oor die spesifieke aard van die verwantskap tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal te verkry, asook of 'n onderliggende faktor, naamlik fonologiese prosesseringsprobleme 'n oorsaak van beide gesproke taalprobleme en geskrewe taalprobleme is, is in die literatuur geidentifiseer (van kleeck, 1990 en 1992; kamhi & catts, 1991). dit blyk egter sinvol te wees om resente vraagstukke rakende gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme binne die leergestremde populasie te ondersoek, aangesien leergestremde kinders 'n hoe-risikopopulasie is om met beide gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme te presenteer (reid, hresko & swanson, 1991; klecan-aker & kelty, 1990). reid et al. (1991) ondersteun die standpunt dat navorsing oor die leergestremde populasie ten spyte van uitgebreide studie steeds geregverdig word. hul meen dat beperkte aandag aan basiese navorsing en aan die ontwikkeling van teoriee geskenk word, en dat navorsing oor leergestremdheid uiters swak georganiseer is. daar word voorgestel dat intensiewe gekoordineerde wetenskaplike navorsing sal bydra tot die oplossing van fundamentele probleme rakende leergestremdheid (reid et al., 1991). nye, foster en seaman (1987) beklemtoon die belang van navorsing, asook die waarde van intervensie by die leergestremde populasie. hul motiveer hul siening deur wetenskaplik te bewys dat taalleergestremde kinders se taalfunksionering merkwaardige verbetering getoon het nadat 'n intensiewe taalintervensieprogram met hulle gevolg is. dit blyk dat die taalvaardigheidsvlakke van die taalleergestremde kinders betekenisvolle verbetering getoon het, en dat navorsing op hierdie spesifieke populasie soveel te meer sinvol blyk te wees wanneer daar na hul vorderingsmoontlikhede gekyk word. beroepsfunksies van die spraak-taalterapeut impliseer die hantering van kommunikasieafwykings (uys, 1993), kommunikasie-afwykings sluit onder andere taalprobleme in wat presenteer op beide gesproke taalvlak en geskrewe taalvlak. wanneer fonologiese prosesseringsvaardighede as voorvereiste vir effektiewe funksionering op geskrewe taalvlak en gesproke taalvlak beskou word (stackhouse, 1990), word fonologiese prosessering onomwonde deel van kommunikasie-afwykings wat onder andere deur die spraak-taalterapeut hanteer word. alhoewel die spraak-taalterapeut as lid van die transprofessionele span betrokke is by evaluering, diagnose en intervensie van leergestremde leerlinge, word die spraaktaalterapieberoep as sodanig met kommunikasieafwykings (wat kommunikasie-afwykings van leergestremdes insluit) geassosieer. die doel van hierdie studie is gevolglik om meer inligting in te win aangaande die aard van die verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taalvermoens van leergestremde leerlinge aangesien dit waardevolle kliniese riglyne vir evaluering, diagnosering en intervensie by die leergestremde populasie verskaf. meer effektiewe bemoeienis rakende gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme kan uit die beskikbaarheid van hierdie inligting voortvloei. metode as agtergrond tot die ondersoek is doelstellings geformuleer. doel die doel van die ondersoek is om die funksionering van 'n groep taalleergestremde leerlinge in onderskeidelik gesproke taalfunksionering, geskrewe taalfunksionering en fonologiese prosessering te ondersoek asook om die aard van die verband tussen gesproke taalvermoens, geskrewe taalvermoens en fonologiese prosesseringsvermoens te bepaal. die volgende subdoelstellings is geformuleer om die doel te bereik: 1. identifikasie van die onderskeie gesproke taalparameters, geskrewe taalparameters en fonologiese prosesseringsparameters. i 2. bepaling van proefpersone se peil van funksionering op die onderskeie parameters. 3. ondersoek na die verwantskap tussen die onderskeie parameters. navorsingsontwerp hierdie studie is in die vorm van 'n kwalitatiewe en kwantitatiewe gestruktureerde beskrywende opname uitgevoer (leedy, 1989). / proefpersone proefpersone is d e u / m i d d e l van die beskikbaarheidseleksie (leedy, 1989) gekies en het aan die volgende kriteria voldoen: the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ο) s> bo φ 73 ci φ ο μ >. pq τ 3 2 ι 's ϊη u 00 α * .2 ο. 0q φ γ" i* fe φ 00 φ 3 ft φ λ xi tη φ ώ φ ·γη 2 α. s 0 1 ft φ cq -β p j e verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van leergestremdes afrikaans moet die moedertaal of dominante taal wees. ' fyj o r m ale visuele en ouditiewe vermoens moet voorkom. 3 s e r e b r a l e gestremdheid mag teenwoordig wees. 4 spesifieke leergestremdheid moet by alle proefpersone gediagnoseer wees. 5 p r o e f p e r s o n e moet graad twee skoliere wees in 'n skool ' vir buitengewone onderwys. g p r o e f p e r s o n e moet oor ' n gemiddelde o f bo-gemiddelde intelligensie beskik. 'n ibtale aantal van nege proefpersone is in hierdie empiriese studie betrek. die inligting vir proefpersoonbeskrywing word volledig in tabel 1 saamgevat. materiaal enapparaat die apparaat en materiaal word in drie afsonderlike afdelings bespreek, naamlik: 1. meetinstrumente om gesproke ttial te evalueer meetinstrumente wat geselekteer is om gesproke taal te evalueer word opsommend in tabel 2 weergegee. 2. meetinstrumente om fonologiese prosessering te evalueer aangesien daar nog geen gestandaardiseerde toets bestaan wat fonologiese prosesseringsvermoens evalueer nie, het die navorser na deeglike literatuurverkenning (van kleeck, 1992; stackhouse, 1990) 'n eie toets ontwerp (vergelyk bylae a), wat spesifiek die segmentering van die akoestiese stroom evalueer. die toets bestaan uit drie subtoetse, naamlik subtoets een wat die segmentering van 'n sin in sy afsonderlike woordkomponente evalueer, subtoets twee wat die segmentering van 'n woord in sy afsonderlike sillabes evalueer en subtoets drie wat die segmentering van 'n woord in sy afsonderlike klankkompohente evalueer. die subtoetse is so geformuleer dat verskillende klankkombinasies wat in afrikaans voorkom, deur die subtoetse gedek word. ! i 3. meetinstrumente om geskrewe taal te evalueer meetinstrumente wat geselekteer is om geskrewe taal te evalueer word opsommend in tabel 3 weergegee. opname prosedures die prosedure is soos volg toegepas: 1. data-insamelingsprosedure die volgende prosedure is gevolg: toestemming is van die skoolhoof verkry om die betrokke empiriese studie op die graad twee leerlinge uit te voer. " 'n profielblad met identifiserende inligting rakende alle proefpersone is deur die navorser vir elke proefpersoon voltooi. 1 55 φ s => a ο ο 5"® vh φ β fls « ύ s a ο cu w ^ i—i « bfl i l l λ s ® > 2 ω rt ci • s 3 -μ '3 ^ s h i-i ο ĵ +j φ bo bo bj δ ft ci cη μ g cq bo ϋ ci ci 3 ο a-s a nj o j u1 ϋ ci i φ φ .g -aϊ co < j ο 2 ci •γη ιa bo ci ••e β •a bj 73 ci bj < ci φ δ h£ φ φ i — i α φ λ ft μ < 3 3 tf ο (ν (ν co (ν ο ο 00 ιο ο ο (ν 00 00 (ν 00 <35 (ν ο 00 κ κ κ die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 proefpersone is aan spraak-taalterapeute en 'n remedierende onderwyseres toegeken vir uitvoering van diagnostiese toetsing. spesifieke voorskrifte is gevolg ten opsigte van uitvoering van toetse. akkurate beskrywing van data is op gefotostateerde vorms aangeteken. 'n statistikus is geraadpleeg vir dataverwerking. idilette oosthuizen, brenda louw en isabel uy 2. data-analise om doelstelling een te bereik is alle veranderlikes in dj data-definisie gedefinieer (vergelyk bylae b) e* gekategoriseer onder gesproke taalparameters, geskre^ taalparameters en fonologiese prosesseringsparameters om die tweede doelstelling te bereik is die volgend stappe beplan (leedy, 1989): 6 tabel 2: meetinstrumente wat geselekteer is om gesproke taal te evalueer evaluasie-area meetinstrument motivering vir keuse van toets bron i taalinhoud * peabody picture and vocabu lary test (ppvt) (vertaald( afrikaanse weergawe) * die ppvt gee 'n verbale intelligensiekwosien! wat effektief met verbale tellings, soos gemeet deur die jsais (madge, 1981) of ssais (madge, 1983) vergelyk kan word. die ppvt is verder geselekteer aangesien die afrikaanse reseptiewe woordeskattoets (arw), nog nie beskikbaar is nie. * oorspronklike samesteller -lloyd, 1959 * test for auditory comprehension of language (tacl-r) (vertaalde afrikaanse weergawe) * inligting met betrekking tot die proefpersoon se reseptiewe taal word in 'n kort tyd verkry. inligting behels naamlik woordklasse en relasies, grammatikale morfeme en uitgebreide sinne. 'n ouderdomspeil word verkry wat intertoetsrelasies en interproefpersoonrelasies vergemaklik. * oorspronklike samesteller -carrow-woolfolk, 1985 ii taalvorm * die afrikaanse semantiese taal-evalueringsmedium (ast) -subtoets 2: woorddefinisies * die ast is 'n resente toets wat ekspressiewe uitinge (woordbeskrywing) op 'n maklike en informele wyse evalueer. die omskakeling van die roupunt na 'n ontwikkelingsvlak word op 'n tabel afgelees, en langdradige verwerkings van resultate word gevolglik daardeur uitgeskakel. deur middel van die ouderdomsvlak kan effektiewe interproefpersoon en intertoetsrelasies afgelei word. * pretorius, 1989 * toets vir mondelinge taalproduksie (tmt) * die tmt verskaf metings vir 16 fasette van taalproduksie wat saam produktiwiteit, sintaktiese kompleksiteit, korrektheid, vlotheid en inhoud dek. stanege-tellings kan effektief in statistiese vergelykings gebruik word. * vorster, 1980 i iii taalgebruik * pragmatiek-evaluasie * hierdie vraelys bestaan uit 'n kort en bondige metode waardeur verskeie aspekte van pragmatiek geevalueer word. direkte terapieleidrade word verder deur die vraelys verkry. die roupunt uit 20 kan effektief met di6 van ander leerlinge vergelyk word. * creaghead, 1984 ,' iv ouditiewe prosessering * pendulum die volgende areas word getoets, naamlik -ouditiewe storiegeheue, •ouditiewe opeenvolgende geheue, ouditiewe sluiting, ouditiewe analise en ouditiewe sintese. * die pendulum is gekies aangesien die toets maklik uitvoerbaar is. 'n pertinente ouderdomsen standerdpeil word ook verkry, wat effektief in statistiese vergelykings gebruik kan word. tallal (1980 in stackhouse, 1990) beklemtoon ook die verband tussen leesen ouditief-perseptuele probleme, met die gevolg dat ouditiewe prosessering noodwendig deel van die toetsbattery moet uitmaak. "struwig, 1984 / / y the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van minimumen maksimumwaarde word vir alle * n nderlikes bepaal. hprlikes met negatiewe tellings word omgeskakel * v e r a o s i t i e w e tellings sodat hoer toetstellings, beter n a l s i e by die toetsling impliseer. η s k a l e r i n g vind plaas m e t ander woorde, alle * raiiderlikes word getransformeer na 'n nul-eenv t e r v a l s k a a l sodat alle veranderlikes met mekaar m g e l y k b a a r kan wees, en toetsresultate word op 'n u litatiewe wyse beskryf om sodoende die moontlike v e r b a n d e en verklarings vir die verbande uit te wys, al dan nie. om die derde doelstelling te bereik, is die volgende stappe beplan (leedy, 1989): • pearson se parametriese korrelasietegniek word toegepas om sodoende die korrelasie-koeffisient te bepaal. • na aanleiding van die waarde van pearson se korrelasiekoeffisient kan daar bepaal word of betekenisvolle verbande tussen gesproke taalparameters, geskrewe taalparameters en fonologiese prosesseringsparameters bestaan, al dan nie. • s p e a r m a n se nie-parametriese korrelasietegniek word toegepas om sodoende die korrelasiekoeffisient te bepaal na aanleiding van die waarde van spearman se korrelasiekoeffisient wat rangordekorrelasies bepaal. resultate en bespreking die resultate word vervolgens aangebied. leergestremdes 57 i. identifikasie van onder skeie parameters op gesproke taalvlak, geskrewe taalvlak en fonologiese prosesseringsvlak die toepaslike veranderlikes word vir die doel van die studie in tabel 4 weergegee. //. uiteensetting van proefpersoonfunksionering op die onderskeie parameters die parameters wat 'n betekenisvolle verskil met kronologiese ouderdom van die groep proefpersone aandui, word in tabel 5 saamgevat. iii. bespreking enverklaring van die aard van die korrelasies wat tussen gesproke taalparameters, geskrewe taalparameters en fonologiese prosesseringsparameters geidentifiseer is die aard van die korrelasies tussen gesproke taalparameters, geskrewe taalparameters en fonologiese prosesseringsparameters word in tabel 6 saamgevat. alhoewel outeurs verskil oor die spesifieke aard van gesproke taaluitvalle, geskrewe taaluitvalle en fonologiese prosesseringsuitvalle by taalleergestremdes, asook oor die verband tussen hierdie vaardighede, is outeurs en navorsers dit eens dat taalleergestremde leerlinge 'n hoerisiko-populasie is, omdat hulle met uitvalle op hierdie tabel 3: evaluasie-areas en meetinstrumente wat geselekteer is om geskrewe taal te evalueer areas wat geevalueer word meetinstrument motivering vir keuse van toets en afleidings wat gemaak kan word uit die verkree toetsresultate bron i. leesvermoe individuele diagnostiese leestoets (prosalees en begrip) inligting word verkry aangaande 'n leerling se visuele begrip, aangesien hul self'n voorgeskrewe stuk moet lees en daarna mondelinge antwoorde op vrae moet gee. inligting word ook verkry aangaande 'n kind se visuele storiegeheue. grove & hauptfleisch (1984) een-minuutleestoets inligting aangaande die kind se leesspoed word verkry. afleidings kan gemaak word m.b.t. hul sintesevermoe en begrip. leerlinge met 'n hoe ik is geneig om hulself te korrigeer. transvaalse onderwysdepartement (ongedateer) ii. spellingvermoe k3 skolastiese toets bo en behalwe inligting oor 'n leerling se spellingvermoe kan afleidings gemaak word aangaande die kind se begrip van stillees. transvaalse onderwysdepartement (ongedateer) ipv spellingtoets inligting aangaande die leerling se spellingvermoe word verkry in terme van woorde wat progressief moeiliker gegradeer is. universiteit van wes-kaapland (ongedateer) tod taaltoets 1 inligting word verkry aangaande stillees-begrip. inligting word met betrekking tot spelling en begrip verkry aangesien vrae skriftelik beantwoord moet word. transvaalse onderwysdepartement (ongedateer) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 idilette oosthuizen, brenda louw en isabel uy c ο t-pa σι in σι 13 £ σι in σι 13 £ μ § ο 1 i * 2 m 5 °° « os ο ή μ a ο i i * ο η m a °° ο ή ο i ι * ο η in s 0 0 13 c ce φ ce bo tφ φ * cn ce φ co α cd φ ce w φ >>2 ο i •s * cd tdu > 2 ® . 2 ® co s co β c φ 2 λ s φ je a ^ ο φ ο 13 φ bo ce a α cd j φ bo ce a α ce j ω bo ce a α cd hj «τ-ο α ο φ * cd bo th φ φ 2 ω « s φ 2 <2 ce φ μ s ^ ο φ ο ^ "ί « th i co < ώεη. c c φ cd β s φ η χ! « λ s >> cd κ 0 3 ο -υ -s tn th t co < φ λ a ^ ο φ ο ^ is ce co < φ μ a ^ ο φ ο 2 >»"ΐ3 jh cd th • co < ω λ s ^ ο φ o ^ b ce ο • t-£ « 13 in < ρ ρ ρ ρ ρ ρ ρ s s § § § § § e-i ε-ι ε-ι ε-ι ε-ι εη ρ φ φ _φ φ φ 'ω *0q '3 's '3 '3 "3 λ λ λ μ λ λ λ "3 λ ρ 3 ρ ρ 9 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 ο ο ο ο ο ο ο t h t h t h t h t h t h t h j5* j5* α . α , 3 "ce "ce "ce 13 "ce ce cd ce cd ce ce cd ε-ι ε-ι ε-ι ε-ι ε-ι ε-ι η φ ω ω φ φ φ φ bo bo bo bo bo bo bo β β β .β β _β β • t-l *·-ι "•η •η "ω φ φ " φ φ " φ "ω 13 13 13 13 13 13 13 β β β β β β β ο ο ο ο ο ο ο § s § § § § t h t h t h t h t h t h t h 'ρ 'ρ 'ρ 'ρ v co cq co cq co co αϊ -μ φ ω φ ω ω ω φ β β β β a ο 13 th φ 13 ρ ο 03 13 β ce -μ co th c :φ •t-l 03 ο ^ λ φ '«3 β bo a a ι •s a m a φ r 3 0 0 'φ cq m ο bo-s κ 3 £ cc a p φ § • s g φ μ 'φ co φ ρ 92 κ 3 id cο a ρ a a •fn a (μ ω ο cο β ω 13 13 th ο i a ρ a co λ ce a φ ο -μ •9 cο β ω 13 13 th ο i ω 13 th ο ο β ce ce φ̂ ω xi β ω φ t-ω α. φ 13 th ο ο β ce ce "ω 13 13 a ω ο ω xi β φ φ •2 ^ ce th i β ω λ ω ^ ι φ α. & u ω α, φ 13 th ο ο ι pa φ xi β ω ω tφ ο. ω co th ο ί? ω xi β φ ω tφ ο. φ 13 th ο ο i tφ * ω 13 φ bo $ a ^ i -c ο β ο fa co a 03 cd φ tcq cd ο o< ft φ ^ ω t-μ co φ ο ui £ λ ω s s s 2 ce o. s δ ο ' φ ftl s s 2 ο a χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ x β ce is > γ3 φ h u φ· h i δ -1 ^ φ λ > 00 a ο fc ο (μ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die e verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van leergestremdes 59 c ο t-pa ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο 00 00 00 00 00 00 00 00 00 σι σι σι σι σι σι σι σι σι τ-ι τ-ι τ-ι τ-ι τη τ-ι τ-ι τ-ι τ—i tt li tt tn" tn" tn" φ . t j φ φ φ 8 8 ' " 8 8 2 cq co co cq υι αϊ 01 co co lh μ, tu u th u (ν £ £ £ £ ί? £ is i? •ψ 00 σ ι t-® . <§§ ® m > s" 5 1 co ® t 3 •a i i ® £ β β β φ φ φ αϊ ΐω tt ti tn" φ φ φ " 3 > o. 0 0 -s "β > rt β ό φ il lit qpa qpa a φ β s η φ φ ε—< ε—< ε—< § § § ε—< ε—< ε—< ω λ τ 3 ο >73 & φ i " φ τ 3 β ο >φ •fl ω •3 τ 3 ο >-j3" 73 £ φ υο c •fl "φ τ 3 β ο § >ε-ι ε-ι § § b b φ φ 3 -9 1 τ 3 ο >73 & φ i " φ τ 3 β ο >τ 3 ο >73 £ φ υο β τ 3 ο >α. 73 £ φ υο β φ φ φ r o r o _© ε-ι ε-ι ε-ι •3 -s β β ο ο 53 § >>-03 co -w -w φ φ β s ε-ι ε-ι § § ε-ι ε-ι · ® *03 μ τ 3 ο >73 £ φ υο β 1 β ο § >φ μ τ 3 ο >ο. 73 £ φ ω β 1 β ο § >φ ε-ι ε-ι 53 § ε-ι ε-ι φ -t-l co λ τ 3 ο >α. co λ τ 3 ο >ο. 73 & & β "φ τ 3 β ο >φ co j>> φ cs •a 6: ce £ φ ω .β "φ τ 3 β ο >φ a co β •*» a i i s co φ 73 ce φ xi ω ce φ tο 3 ρ β β φ w ω β φ α. cο ι φ ο ο μ cο ρ? φ ο co φ φ co ο a 2 " φ ρ 73 β φ 5 73 ce ^ q ο η οι αϊ ΐω φ φ φ 2 ο ΐω 03 ΐω bo bo bo β β β " β έ χ φ φ φ οι 03 οι οι οι 03 φ φ φ οι οι ΐω ο ο ο £ £ £ φ φ φ οι ΐω οι φ φ φ •a •a •a ο ο " ο " ο ο β β β ο ο ο fa fa >φ -μ φ a ce tce -e· φ μ τη -s β ce th u β ce > ω β u μ co φ pa λ λ uu i β « co ce φ >co ce 2 o. .3 13 φ xi β φ φ φ xi β φ φ >φ α, co •a β ce > t2 2 η φ xi β φ φ >φ ο. co ω β α, >£ t-φ ο. co ω β φ ή 'ο ο "ο > β ο φ xi β φ φ >φ ο. 03 ω β χ φ ^ φ ΐ φ 6 ρ λ i co β •fn cο φ xi β φ ® η εη 3 λλ >φ α. co β5 χ φ φ ΐ φ > φ co ρ φ λ 73 th ο i tφ α. co ω α 73 χι th ^ 73 th ο i φ φ tφ ο. co ω β 1 φ ο β φ pa φ •a φ φ ε̂ tφ ο. μ φ τ3 β φ xi φ φ t7* ο λ i λ ce th β χ ! ο < λ φ ce th cl, 73 φ ο ο. τω co φ φ hj ω β φ α, cο ω β φ ο. cο α, "β φ χ! β φ ΐω φ φ 73 co ο tcl, "β φ χ! φ φ cc φ β β β ® i« ^ φ 69 β β ο •c λ φ a « i i f cο ? φ χ! ce φ τ3 th ο ο ^ β ce > ω β χ φ β φ φ cο φ ce a β •t-l φ 73 th ο ο > ω β χ φ β φ φ cο φ »η ε φ > -w φ η $ h ce β 01 δ ο ι φ & 2 s ω 3 « g ο. β ce ^ is i ί φ > ί 9 β „ ce ο ^ a ο fc (μ (μ co (μ (μ w (μ co (μ (μ 00 (μ σ ι (μ ο co co ιη •ψ co (μ w co w die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 idilette oosthuizen, brenda louw en isabel uy vlakke presenteer (reid et al., 1991; klecan-aker & kelty, 1990; stackhouse, 1990; van kleeck, 1992). volgens die resultate van di6 studie toon slegs ses van die 35 gei'dentifiseerde parameters 'n betekenisvolle verskil op 'n 5% en 10% betekenispeil met die kronologiese ouderdom van die groep proefpersone. hierdie verskille is van so 'n aard dat dit die implikasie het dat die spesifieke groep proefpersone bo hul kronologiese ouderdom presteer met betrekking tot begrip vir woordklasse en verhoudings, begrip vir grammatikale morfeme, totale aantal woorde per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980), woordherhalings per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980), stillees-begrip, en segmentering van 'n sin in afsonderlike woorde. bogenoemde resultate is egter teenstrydig met navorsingsresultate en menings van outeurs wat beweer dat taalprobleme 'n sentrale deel van leergestremdheid, en gevolglik ook van taalleergestremdheid uitmaak (asha, 1982; klecan-aker & kelty, 1990). hierdie a-tipiese resultate bevestig egter die siening van nye et al. (1987) wat beweer dat taalleergestremde kinders se taalfunksionering 'n merkwaardige verbetering kan toon nadat intensiewe taalintervensieprogramme met hulle gevolg is. die vraag ontstaan nou egter of die tydstip waarop vroee intervensie toegepas is, moontlik die funksionering van die proefpersone kon bei'nvloed. die proefpersone in hierdie studie is reeds vanaf driejarige ouderdom in die kleuterafdeling van die betrokke skool ingeskakel. tydens hierdie pre-geletterdheidsfase het die proefpersone reeds spraakterapie, fisioterapie en/ of arbeidsterapie ontvang. die proefpersone het intensiewe langtermyn terapie in die kritieke taalaanleerperiode ontvang. die trans-professionele insette op voorskoolse vlak kon moontlik tot die proefpersone se voordeel gestrek het, aangesien taalopleidingsprogramme soos in hoofstroomonderwys toegepas word, nie noodwendig kompenseer vir die verlies aan stimulasietyd, wat leerlinge met uitvalle wat eers op skoolvlak geidentifiseer is, ondervind nie (cole, 1982). alhoewel verskeie outeurs die waarde van vroee intervensie beklemtoon (asha, 1988; louw, 1990), bestaan daar tans verskille tussen outeurs rakende die siening of hoe-risiko-kinders eventueel met spraaken/of taalprobleme gaan presenteer, al dan nie. menyuk, liebergott, schultz, chesnick en ferrier (1991) het bewys dat daar nie 'n verskil bestaan tussen die taalontwikkeling en kognitiewe ontwikkeling van hoe-risiko-kinders (premature babas) en nie-hoe-risiko-kinders (voltermyn-babas) wat as proefpersone in hul studie gedien het nie. aram, hack, hawkins, weissman en borawski-clark (1991) het bewys dat meer nie-hoe-risiko-kinders (voltermyn-babas) met spraaken taalafwykings presenteer, as kinders in die hoe-risiko-groep. aangesien daar tans meningsverskille in die literatuur en navorsing bestaan rakende die invloed van vroee intervensie op latere ontwikkeling, kan daar nie sonder meer aanvaar word dat die a-tipiese resultate van die proefpersone op van die gesproke taalparameters, geskrewe taalparameters en fonologiese prosesseringsparameters, nie die resultaat is van vroee intervensie nie. oosthuizen (1994) is van mening dat vroee intervensie wel die proefpersoonfunksionering positief bei'nvloed het, in die opsig dat vroee inkortings van spraaken taalontwikkeling, voorlopers vir latere leerprobleme kan wees (aram & nation, 1980). indien die vroee ingryping deur die trans-professionele span nie noodwendig as primgre rede vir die proefpersone se funksionering aangevoer kan word nie, moet hul insette noodwendig die uitkoms van die leerprobleme minimaliseer (nye et al. 1987). aard van die korrelasies wat tussen gesproke taalparameters, geskrewe taalparameters en fonologiese prosesseringsparameters geidentifiseer is. volgens tabel 6 toon die proefpersone se taalbegrip vir woordklasse en relasies 'n positiewe verband met beide stillees-begrip, asook met die vermoe om sinne in afsonderlike woorde te segmenteer. die positiewe korrelasie wat statisties by die genoemde parameters geidentifiseer is, kan moontlik gei'nterpreteer word in die lig daarvan dat begrip vir woordklasse en relasies verband hou met die tipe begripsvrae wat aan die proefpersone gestel is, nadat hulle die stillees-leesstuk voltooi het. indien die proefpersone nie goeie begrip getoon het vir die betekenis van woordklasse en relasies nie, kon hul moontlik 'n inkorting gehad het om sinne in afsonderlike woorde te segmenteer, aangesien die sin eerder as 'n geheelboodskap gei'nterpreteer is, as bestaande uit 'n aantal individuele woorde. alhoewel die verband tussen fonologiese prosessering en gesproke taalfunksionering by hierdie groep proefpersone geidentifiseer is, word verdere navorsing steeds benodig om vas te stel of fonologiese prosessering die oorsaak van gesproke taalfunksionering is, soos deur stackhouse (1990) beweer word. dit blyk dat die proefpersone se vermoe om uitgebreide sinne te begryp 'n positiewe verband met begripsvermoe van 'n prosaleesstuk aangetoon het. hierdie resultate kan moontlik verklaar word na aanleiding daarvan dat indien die proefpersone nie oor die vermoe beskik om 'n enkele uitgebreide sin soos byvoorbeeld: "die man praat met die dogtertjie se ma wat in die kar sit" te verstaan nie, hulle nie begrip sal toon vir 'n prosaleesstuk, wat uit 'n hele aantal uitgebreide sinne saamgestel is nie. woorddefinieringsvermoe het 'n negatiewe korrelasie met leesspoed en fonologiese prosessering getoon. hierdie prestasie kan moontlik verklaar word op grond van die medium (ouditief of visueel) waardeur die proefpersone getoets is, naamlik dat gesproke taal in hierdie geval deur die ouditiewe medium (ouditiewe opdrag), en geskrewe taal deur die visuele medium (geskrewe leesstuk) geevalueer is. dit blyk dat die spesifieke meetinstrument wat gebruik word, die resultate kan bei'nvloed. die negatiewe korrelasie tussen verbale ekspressie en die vermoe om sinne in individuele woorde te segmenteer kan moontlik verklaar word op grond daarvan dat verbale ekspressie in hierdie geval deur die proefpersone se woorddefinieringsvermoe bepaal is. die fonologiese prosesseringsvaardigheid daarenteen, word geevalueer deur vooraf geformuleerde sinne wat deur die remedierende onderwyseres aan die proefpersoon gestel is, waarvan die fonologiese, morfologiese en sintaktiese sinstrukture nie noodwendig aan die proefpersoon bekend is nie. hierdie resultate bevestig die standpunt van stackhouse (1990) en van kleeck (1992), naamlik dat taalleergestremde leerlinge met fonologiese prosesseringsprobleme kan presenteer. dit blyk dat die proefpersone 'n omgekeerde verband toon tussen die totale aantal woorde wat in ekspressiewe taal gebruik word en stillees-begrip. taalleergestremde leerlinge toon dikwels ernstige en permanente uitvalle in die begrip en gebruik van meer abstrakte taal (myers & the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van leergestremdes 61 0 ο ο -*-» a ο ό h φ ό 9 ο 0) χ » •a ο ο α ο 0) a £ •3 α 0) ό ·ρ4 9 ό » 42 1 ε 0) 0) a 2 α ft, «ο ι-ί w a st at is ti es be du id en d op 'n 5 % p ei l +0 ,0 31 3 st at is tie s be du id en d op 'n 1 0% p ei l va n be te ke ni s +0 ,0 74 2 +0 ,0 54 7 +0 ,0 54 7 +0 ,0 74 2 +0 ,0 74 2 +0 ,0 54 7 f on ol og ie se pr os es se ri ng spa ra m et er va n be te ke ni s g es kr ew e ta al pa ra m et er x g es pr ok e ta al pa ra m et er x x χ x b es ki yw in g va n m ee ti ns tr um en t pa ra m et er b eg ri p vi r te st f or a ud it or y c om pr eh en si on o f l an gu ag e (t a c l -r ) w oo rd kl as se v er ta al de a fr ik aa ns e w ee rg aw e (c ar ro w -w oo lfo lk , 19 85 ) en v er ho ud in gs b eg ri p vi r te st f or a ud it or y c om pr eh en si on o f l an gu ag e (t a c l -r ) gr am m at ik al e v er ta al de a fr ik aa ns e w ee rg aw e (c ar ro w -w oo lfo lk , 19 85 ) m or fe m e to ta le a an ta l to et s vi r m on de lin ge t aa lp ro du ks ie ( t m t ) ( v or st er , 19 80 ) w oo rd e in ek sp re ss ie w e ta al w oo rd he rh al in gs to et s vi r m on de lin ge t aa lp ro du ks ie ( t m t ) (v or st er , 19 80 ) pe r tee nh ei d pr ag m at ie k pr ag m at ie kv ra el ys ( c re ag he ad , 19 84 ) st ill ee sbe gr ip to d t aa lt oe ts (t o d , on ge da te er ) se gm en te ri ng v an fo no lo gi es e pr os es se ri ng st oe ts (d ie s kr yw er , si en b y la e a ) si nn e in w oo rd ko m po ne nt e «β a "ο μ φ * φ (β a ο ο μ α> φ α> a ο ο μ φ φ a «β ϋ ο. φ ο. •ρ* η ί» v η αϊ β e h .§ ® a "s © β ® οί λ s © h ο u β fe ο, ο, 2 · £ (β ® β3 ^ ft ® 8τ3 ® « 5 8 ® λ jd « ο b h 2 h ft ft ® s i ® ϋ 5 a χ x x a ο ** ο μ 2 > a ο ο μ φ λ μ μ ® c ? λ 3 5 «β i * α .9 w •α £ λ αϊ ιη ο ο 2 ο a s · ο ,3 » s 6 « t> in co in co th ο ο ο ο tf tf (μ ο a ο ο μ φ φ * a ο ο μ φ ό (β * φ α> φ έ φ a i .2 β «ι « is 1 bs ® ο ο b λ λ co oo co tf t> oo σι co co t> ο ο fl 2 α 5 » :« a « a ® ε « ft ο ο 03 ji λ ο 00 co co t> fl φ φ a ο ο μ α> φ jd •w4 ΐ φ ό fl « i α> h φ -*-» φ a (β u ce & « φ a φ φ αϊ αϊ 3 « >: ω is b a. si ·&_φ a ' f s s φ ce = λ -s3 η co φ ό s § s £ ί § ϋ ce •φ 73 w φ φ c s & s 2 s a 1 ? 's a § c sp 3 > φ ό • r h ® v φ ^ sp « '3 <35 > ft "so of a ο § c co φ φ a si j a £ > 3 φ co .s eh ό : φ ο a ι* φ > co α. "fi φ μ die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 idilette oosthuizen, brenda louw en isabel uyg α a ο 3 « a jg "o μ « m a ο i tn v a !β is α ν α ρ 1— 09 « μ v £5 g s s ο «ι j j ts 4) β fl ο 2 «3 fa a α s • £ g fl) η δ rt u •ίί-βί $ λ β a ® • -μ λ ο it h ! h α α α) δ 2 ® a w ο χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ χ a ja ο μ u a jg ο μ φ μ μ •fn • s i 00 "β co μ 2 8 » * tf μ ib h ο ο τ-η (ν τ-η ι—ι > > co oj co τ-η co ο ο θ" ο* •ψ co co ο 00 co a,® a «•a λ 8 s w h > 1—' '—· ι> l> (ν (ν (ν (ν ο ® θ" θ" 10 os i—l •ψ co co 0 0 0 0 00 ο •ψ ο > •—· ν / co co (ν 00 (ν ιο ιο ο t> t> ο" ο" ιο co 00 00 in 00 ο 00 co co 00 0 0 a .2 fl rt 5 :φ a ® s ® & φ α ο ο go -μ -μ t-h i n i l 1-1 > > ο ο 00 co co cjl cj1 co co co •ψ 0 0 0 co 00 th α 73 ® c r 1 § > φ α · 3 ' 6 φ ε-ι ^ ^ β φ φ ft m ό ο ^ s α ι ' 5 * ce φ α* * s ce ® ά ® 2 ® ® 73 tφ α. φ -β ιη ο s· ο co £ ^ β >-. φ s ® ι > φ 0 φ _ φ s ^ β s 3 ft φ s ι φ ιο s φ 3 ® μ c c φ ο , q. (β η φ ce φ ^ β β ο " φ i l l φ * •s φ bs s a g ο _ ο· α m a 3 ® ο < τ3 μ ce ih φ ft co ,-ν bp co 5 ^ rt φ b o 0 s § —i ' 2 t3 β s 2 ο 5 ο· to ο ί-α, β φ • d φ φ χ co ε-ι ^ ^ β fts a l β > ij5 3 s g s αϊ bo β ν φ ^ 2 ® js.g φ c-2 i ι :φ & ο φ φ >· αϊ β rs φ αϊ :φ φ 2 * η 'ή ε > 3 co β bo ο β ,® c ο3 j . s β τ-η ο ιο •co 1 u3 φ β ίο κ φ φ 5 (ν ιο β ε ό 9 c φ > φ φ 3 2 c ο ® i bo ? β φ •μ ^ φ τι ft φ oj τ) α ο _ s ώ ιο .s s "s ό ο !> > ι* c φ i s ι ι gs -θ λ β ^ φ α bo ® β φ a μ ft φ oj ί3 ιο β "" c— φ j f β > β 5 φ -τ3 β s ο. i i , a φ s3 •9 "β co φ φ " β s § oj c3 φ s •3 i φ a ό φ ti. φ φ ce > ^ φ « > β ϋ w * the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van leergestremdes die tfammill, 1990). tydens evaluering van die proefpersone b e g r i p ' v a n ' n stillees-leesstuk word beide konkrete en s® t r a k t e taal as deel van die toetsinhoud voorgele. die 2 ole is dat 'n diskrepansie tussen die toetsprestasies van die p r o e f p e r s o n e op hierdie onderskeie twee vaardighede kan bestaan, aangesien spontane gesprekvoering deur middel van konkrete taal kan geskied, terwyl die stilleesb e g r i p s t o e t s uit beide konkrete en abstrakte taalelemente kan bestaan. dit blyk dat die proefpersone se funksionering 'n n o s i t i e w e verband tussen die gemiddelde aantal woorde wat hul gebruik het, en hul begrip vir 'n prosa-leesstuk a a n g e t o o n het. hierdie korrelasie blyk sinvol te wees want hoe groter die woordeskat van die proefpersone is, hoe meer woorde behoort hulle in die prosa-leesstuk te begryp. v a n w e e die positiewe aard van die korrelasie, bei'nvloed hierdie twee vermoens mekaar wedersyds. dit blyk dat die proefpersone se gesproke taalvermoe 'n verbetering sal toon indien g e s k r e w e taal 'n v e r b e t e r i n g toon en o m g e k e e r d . volgens die resultate van die empiriese ondersoek blyk dit dat daar 'n positiewe verband bestaan tussen die aantal mede-werkwoorde wat die proefpersone per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980) gebruik en spoedleesvermoe, asook tussen die aantal mede-werkwoorde wat die proefpersone per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980) gebruik en die proefpersone se vermoe om sinne in onderlinge woorde te segmenteer. 'n moontlike verklaring vir die positiewe korrelasie tussen die aantal mede-werkwoorde wat die proefpersone per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980) gebruik het en hul spoedleesvermoe, kan wees dat werkwoorde dikwels die kernwoord van 'n sin vorm. 'n goeie leser lees gevolglik nie noodwendig elke woord in die sin, om die voile boodskap te begryp nie (leaf, 1994). aangesien die proefpersone goeie begrip vir mede-werkwoorde toon, en hul spontane spraak as sodanig gekenmerk word deur die gebruik van mede-werkwoorde, kan hul spoedleesvermoe moontlik hierdeur bevoordeel word, deurdat hul in staat is om die mede-werkwoorde en werkwoorde in 'n leesstuk of individueel, te lees en te begryp. 'n moontlike interpretasie van die geidentiflseerde positiewe korrelasie wat bepaal is tussen die aantal mede-werkwoorde wat die proefpersone per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980) gebruik het en hul vermoe om sinne jin onderlinge woorde te segmenteer, kan wees dat die gebruik van werkwoorde en mede-werkwoorde alreeds 'n hoer linguistiese vaardigheid impliseer as met die gebruik van selfstandige naamwoorde (myers & hammill, 1990). aangesien die proefpersone alreeds hierdie vlak van linguistiese v a a r d i g h e i d bemeester het, is hul in staat om te begryp dat 'n sin uit afsonderlike woordkomponente bestaan wat eventueel 'n kommunikasieboodskap weergee. volgens hierdie ondersoek (tabel 6) blyk dit dat 'n negatiewe korrelasie geidentifiseer is tussen die aantal woorde wat die proefpersone per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980) weglaat en leesspoed en die aantal woorde wat die proefpersoon per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980) weglaat en begrip vir 'n prosa-leesstuk. dit blyk dat hoe minder woorde die proefpersone per t-eenheid (tmt: vorster, 1980) weggelaat het, hoe beter hul leesvermoe was. hoe beter 'n persoon se taalvaardigheid ontwikkel het, hoe minder sal weglatings van woorde in spontane spraak plaasvind. aangesien 'n persoon met min of geen weglatings van woorde in spontane spraak, alreeds 'n gevorderde vlak van linguistiese ontwikkeling bereik het 63 (myers & hammill, 1990), blyk dit moontlik te wees dat die genoemde vaardigheid verband kan hou met leesspoed, aangesien leesspoed ook 'n bepaalde vlak van linguistiese en visueel-perseptuele maturasie vereis. net so kan die aantal woorde wat 'n proefpersoon in spontane spraak weglaat met begrip vir 'n prosa-leesstuk verband hou. hoe beter 'n persoon se woordeskat ontwikkel is, hoe minder woorde sal hul in spontane spraak weglaat, en hoe beter kan hul begrip moontlik vir 'n prosa-leesstuk wees, aangesien hul bekend is met 'n goed ontwikkelde woordeskatkennis. alvorens die proefpersone in staat kan wees om selfkorreksies in spontane spraak te kon toepas, moes hul noodwendig tot 'n gevorderde vlak van linguistiese oordeel ontwikkel het, om tussen korrekte en foutiewe uitinge te kon diskrimineer. net so verg die vermoe om woorde in afsonderlike klankkomponente in te deel 'n gesofistikeerde a a n v o e l i n g vir die k o m p o n e n t e w a a r u i t 'n w o o r d saamgestel is (van kleeck, 1992). volgens die resultate (tabel 6) blyk dit dat daar 'n positiewe verband bestaan tussen die proefpersone se abstraheringsvermoe en hul spellingvermoe en die proefpersone se abstraheringsvermoe en hul vermoe om woorde in afsonderlike sillabes te segmenteer. die rede dat die proefpersone se abstraheringsvermoe met beide hul s p e l l i n g v e r m o e en hul v e r m o e om w o o r d e in afsonderlike sillabes te segmenteer geassosieer word, kan moontlik verklaar word na aanleiding van die verskillende ontwikkelingstadia wat alreeds in 1953 deur piaget beskryf is (myers & hammill, 1990). die vermoe om op 'n abstrakte vlak te funksioneer impliseer 'n gevorderde vlak van kognitiewe en linguistiese funksionering. die resultate van die studie lewer voldoende bewys dat daar 'n verband tussen gesproke taalfunksionering, geskrewe taalfunksionering en fonologiese prosessering by die proefpersone bestaan. verskeie verklarings kan aangevoer word vir die geidentiflseerde korrelasies, naamlik vlak van linguistiese maturasie, bewussyn van fonologiese struktuur van taal, modaliteitsvoorkeur (visueel of ouditief) en die tipe voorkennis wat benodig word alvorens 'n bepaalde vaardigheid uitgevoer kan word. net so impliseer 'n goed ontwikkelde spellingvermoe, asook die vermoe om woorde in afsonderlike sillabes te segmenteer, dat die proefpersoon reeds 'n bewussyn gekweek het van die onderliggende fonologiese strukture w a a r u i t taal b e s t a a n (van k l e e c k , 1992). die geidentiflseerde korrelasies hou dus beide verband met hoer taalfunksies, naamlik abstraheringsvermoe en die vermoe om taal in onderlinge eenhede te segmenteer. volgens die resultate (tabel 6) blyk dit dat daar 'n positiewe korrelasie bestaan tussen spellingvermoe en die vermoe om sinne in afsonderlike woorde te segmenteer. hierdie resultate bevestig die siening van outeurs (stackhouse, 1990; kamhi & catts, 1991) dat geskrewe t a a l v e r m o e en fonologiese p r o s e s s e r i n g s v e r m o e interverwant is. hierdie korrelasie hou moontlik verband daarmee dat spellingvermoe as sodanig, 'n vermoe impliseer wat die analise en sintese van grafo-foneme behels (westby & costlow, 1991). hierdie vermoe hou verband met die vermoe van die proefpersone om sinne in afsonderlike woorde te kan segmenteer. beide hierdie vermoens hou verband met 'n eksplisiete kennis van taal, naamlik 'n bewussyn van die fonologiese kennis, sowel as 'n aanvoeling vir foneem-grafeem en alfabetiese kennis (westby & costlow, 1991). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 idilette oosthuizen, brenda louw en isabel uys dit blyk dat daar 'n positiewe verband is tussen begrip vir 'n prosa-leesstuk en die vermoe om 'n sin in afsonderlike woordkomponente te segmenteer (tabel 6). die positiewe korrelasie wat tussen begrip vir 'n prosa-leesstuk en die vermoe om sinne in afsonderlike woordkomponente te segmenteer verkry is, dui eerstens daarop dat, indien die proefpersone 'n voorgeskrewe stuk self gelees het en vrae rakende die leesstuk korrek beantwoord het, hul ook vaardig is om sinne in afsonderlike woordkomponente te segmenteer. hierdie korrelasie kan moontlik verklaar word deurdat beide hierdie parameters met geheue verband hou (grove & hauptfleisch, 1984; van kleeck, 1992). in 'n poging om vrae rakende die prosa-leesstuk te kan beantwoord, moes die proefpersone oor 'n goeie visuele storiegeheue beskik, net so moes die proefpersone oor goeie ouditiewe geheue beskik tydens die fonologiese prosesseringstoets, aangesien 'n gesproke sin op ouditiewe vlak herroep moes word, alvorens die sin in woordkomponente verdeel kon word. gevolgtrekkings en aanbevelings die resultate van hierdie ondersoek lewer voldoende bewys dat daar betekenisvolle korrelasies tussen gesproke taalfunksionering en geskrewe taalfunksionering van die proefpersone bestaan. hierdie inligting noop die spraak-taalterapeut om die grense van haar dienslewering in heroenskou te neem, in die opsig dat gesproke taalprobleme nie in isolasie aangespreek kan word nie. 'n moontlike oplossing vir hierdie probleem kan die volgende insluit: die opleiding van die spraak-taalterapeut behoort verbreed te word na 'n in-diepte-opleiding rakende intervensie van alle kommunikasievaardighede wat beide gesproke en geskrewe taalprobleme impliseer. 'n noue trans-professionele benadering behoort gevolg te word, waar die spraak-taalterapeut, remedierende onderwyseres, arbeidsterapeut en opvoedkundige sielkundige as span saam die taalleergestremde kind hanteer. die proefpersoonresultate lewer bewys van die verband tussen fonologiese prosessering met beide gesproke en geskrewe taal. alhoewel die resultate van di6 studie 'n korrelasie tussen fonologiese prosessering en gesproke en geskrewe taalfunksionering bewys het, kan daar nie sonder meer aangeneem word dat fonologiese prosessering die onderliggende oorsaak vir gesproke en geskrewe taalfunksionering is, soos deur stackhouse (1990) beweer word nie. die korrelasie wat in die empiriese studie tussen fonologiese prosessering en gesproke en geskrewe taal bepaal is, noodsaak die spraak-taalterapeut om fonologiese prosessering as deel van die toetsbattery van geletterdheidsfunksionering by die taalleergestremde kind te implementeer. aangesien geen gestandaardiseerde evalueringsmedium tans bestaan om fonologiese prosessering in afrikaans te evalueer nie, behoort toekomstige navorsing daarop gerig te wees om die ontwikkeling van 'n gestandaardiseerde toetsbattery vir fonologiese prosessering daar te stel. in die lig van die voorafgaande gevolgtrekkings word die volgende aanbevelings geformuleer, naamlik: alhoewel die resultate die korrelasie tussen fonologiese prosessering en geletterdheidsfunksionering by die proefpersone bevestig, word verdere navorsing steeds benodig om te bepaal of fonologiese prosessering as basiese voorvereiste vir geletterdheidsfunksionering beskou kan word, soos hipoteties deur stackhouse (1990) beweer word. die ontwerp en ontwikkeling van kultuurvrye gestandaardiseerde evalueringsmedia, waardeur fonologiese prosessering volledig geevalueer word, behoort daadwerklike aandag in navorsing te geniet. evalueringsmedia behoort dus voorsiening te maak vir die multikulturele en multilinguistiese opset in die rsa. daar word aanbeveel dat fonologiese prosessering by intervensie van die taalleergestremde kind ingesluit word om as deel van kommunikasievaardighede aangespreek te word (catts, 1991). aangesien resente publikasies, asook bevindinge van di£ empiriese studie, die noue verwantskap tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal erken en bevestig, behoort die kurrikulum waarvolgens spraak-taalterapeute opgelei word in heroenskou geneem te word, om te verseker dat in-diepte kennis rakende intervensie van geskrewe taalprobleme as deel van die opleidingsprogram realiseer. navorsing binne die multikulturele en multilinguistiese domein word aanbeveel, sodat wetenskaplik gefundeerde intervensiebeplanning kan geskied met betrekking tot die hantering van geletterdheidsfunksionering en fonologiese prosessering van die bevolking van die rsa. alhoewel hierdie studie die evaluering van gesproke taal, geskrewe taal en fonologiese prosessering beklemtoon, behoort verdere navorsing daarop gerig te wees om terapeutiese hulpverleningsprogramme daar te stel. hierdie programme behoort daarop gerig te wees om habilitasie en rehabilitasie van gesproke taalprobleme, geskrewe taalprobleme en fonologiese prosesseringsprobleme van taalleergestremde leerlinge te bewerkstellig. bibliografie american speech-language-hearing association ](1988). prevention of communication disorders: position statement. american speech and hearing association, 30(3), 90.| american-speech-language-hearing association (committee on language learning disabilities) (1982). "position statement on language-learning disorders." american speech and hearing association, 24(11), 937-944. aram, d.m., hack, m., hawkins, s., weissman b.m. & borawskiclark, e. (1991). very-low-birthweight children and speech and language development. journal of speech and hearing research, 34(5), 1169-1172. aram, d. & nation, j. (1980). preschool language disorders and subsequent language and academic difficulties. journal of communication disorders, 13, 159-170. carrow-woolfolk, e. (1985). test for auditory comprehension of language (tacl-r). texas: dlm, teaching resource. catts, h.w. (1991). facilitating phonological awareness: role of the speech-language pathologists. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 12, 196-203. / cole, p.r. (1982). language disorders in preschool children. new jersey: prentice-hall. creaghead, n.a. (1984). strategies for evaluating and targeting pragmatic behaviors in young children. seminars in speech and language 5(9), 241-250. fick, m.l. (1939). an individual scale of general intelligence for the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e verband tussen g e s p r o k e e n g e s k r e w e taal van leergestremdes 65 south africa. council for educational and social research. pretoria: human sciences research council. grove, m.c. & hauptfleisch, h.m.a.m. (1984). remedierende onderwys in die primere skool. pretoria: haum. j o h n s o n , d.j. & myklebust, h.r. (1967). "learning disabilities. educational principles and practices" in: kamhi, a.g. & catts, h.w. (1991). reading disabilities. a developmental language perspective. united states: little brown and company. kamhi, a.g. & catts, h.w. (1991). reading disabilities. a developmental language perspective. united states: little brown and company. k l e c a n a k e r , j.s. & kelty, k.r. (1990). an investigation of the oral narratives of normal and language-learning disabled children. journal of childhood communication disorders, 13(2), 207-216. leaf, c. (1994). "mind mapping approach." seminaar: pretoria. leedy, p.d. (1989). practical research. planning and design. united states of america: macmillan publishing company. lloyd, m.d. (1959). peabody picture vocabulary test. minnesota: ags. louw, b. (1990). hosrisiko-kinders. vroee programme blyk nodig. bulletin, 2(1), 4-5. madge, e.m. (1981). junior suid-afrikaanse individuele skaal. pretoria: raad vir geesteswetenskaplike navorsing. menyuk, p., liebergott, j., schultz, m., chesnick, m. & ferrier, l. (1991). patterns of early lexical and cognitive development in premature and full-term infants. journal of speech and hearing research, 34(1), 88-94. myers, p.i. & hammill, d.d. (1990). learning disabilities. austin, itexas: pro-ed. nye, c., foster, s.h. & seaman, d. (1987). effectiveness of language intervention with the language/learning disabled." journal of speech and hearing disorders, 52(4), 348-357. oosthuizen, j.i. (1994). die verband tussen gesproke en geskrewe taal van leergestremdes. ongepubliseerde m(log)verhandeling, universiteit van pretoria. pretorius, a. (1989). die afrikaanse semantiese taalevalueringsmedium. pretoria: a. pretorius. reid, d.k., hresko, w.p. & swanson, h.l. (1991). a cognitive approach to learning disabilities. texas: pro-ed. savstg. (1990). "the role of the speech-language-hearing therapist in the assessment, remediation, planning and management in language-learning individuals." savstg seminaar: universiteit van pretoria. stackhouse, j. (1990). phonological deficits in developmental reading and spelling disorders. in p. grunwell (ed.), developmental speech disorders. singapore: longman singapore publishers. struwig, p. (1984). handleiding van die pendulum-toets vir ouditiewe persepsie en konseptualisering. pretoria. transvaalse onderwysdepartement. (ongedateer). een-minuutleestoets; k3 skolastiese toets; tod taaltoets. transvaalse onderwysdepartement: pretoria. universiteit van wes-kaapland. (ongedateer). ipv: spelevalueringstoets. kaapprovinsie: universiteit van weskaapland. uys, i.c. (1993). kommunikasiepatologie:. onderrig vir die toekoms. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 40, 3-9. van kleeck, a. (1990). emergent literacy: learning about print before learning to read. topics in language disorders, 10(2), 25-45. van kleeck, a. (1992). "language assessment and intervention in children: an overview and special focus on preliteracy." saslha: workshop. department of speech pathology and audiology. university of pretoria. vorster, j. (1980). toets vir mondelinge taalproduksie. pretoria: raad vir geesteswetenskaplike navorsing. westby, c.e. & costlow, l. (1991). implementing a whole language program in a special education class. topics in language disorders, 11(3), 69-84. bylae a fonologiese prosesseringstoets s u b t o e t s 1 s e g m e n t e r i n g v a n sinne in w o o r d k o m p o n e n t e 1. sy sit die brood op die tafel. (antwoord: 7) i 2. water tap uit 'n kraan. (antwoord: 5) 3. die druiwe is op die stoof. (antwoord: 6) 4. die olifant, perd en kat word deur die zebra gejaag. j (antwoord: 10) j 5. sy voer die b a b a m e t die lepel. (antwoord: 7) 6. die e m m e r is vol klippe. (antwoord: 5) 7. die seuntjie drink. (antwoord: 3) 8. h y spring op sy fiets. (antwoord: 5) 9. die gogga eet blare. (antwoord: 4) 10. die skoene kos baie geld. (antwoord: 5) 11. h y speel gholf. (antwoord: 3) punte behaal maksimum punte 11 s u b t o e t s 2 s u b t o e t s 3 s e g m e n t e r i n g v a n w o o r d e s e g m e n t e r i n g v a n i n s i l l a b e s w o o r d e i n k l a n k e 1. vurk (antwoord: 1) 1. j a s (antwoord: 3) 2. kwas (antwoord: 1) 2. prop (antwoord: 4) 3. piesang (antwoord: 2) 3. huis (antwoord: 3) 4. sjokolade (antwoord: 4) 4. glas (antwoord: 4) 5. b o o m (antwoord: 1) 5. trein (antwoord: 4) 6. knoop (antwoord: 1) 6. gras (antwoord: 4) 7. vliegtuig (antwoord: 2) 7. swem (antwoord: 4) 8. seep (antwoord: 1) 8. boks (antwoord: 4) 9. spons (antwoord: 1) 9. berg (antwoord: 4) 10. vingers (antwoord: 2) 11. rok (antwoord: 1) 12. stryk (antwoord: 1) 13. masjien (antwoord: 2) 14. neus (antwoord: 1) 15. skryf (antwoord: 1) 16. mielie (antwoord: 2) 17. skrik (antwoord: 1) 18. pleister (antwoord: 2) 19. sleutels (antwoord: 2) 20. wolke (antwoord: 2) punte behaal m a k s i m u m punte 20 punte b e h a a l : m a k s i m u m punte die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 bylae β: data-definisie idilette oosthuizen, brenda louw en isabel u y s nommer van beskrywing van veranderlik gebied waarin kolomme veranderlike roupunt kan voorkom veranderlike 1 respondentnommer 1-9 01 veranderlike 2 kaartnommer 1 02 veranderlike 3 verstandsouderdom (ppvt) 067-149 03-05 veranderlike 4 intelligensiekwosient (ppvt) 070-126 06-08 veranderlike 5 subtoets 1 minimum (tacl-r) 069-097 09-11 veranderlike 6 subtoets 1 maksimum (tacl-r) 083-119 12-14 veranderlike 7 subtoets 2 minimum (tacl-r) 082-113 15-17 veranderlike 8 subtoets 2 maksimum (tacl-r) 095-119 18-20 veranderlike 9 subtoets 3 minimum (tacl-r) 066-119 21-23 veranderlike 10 subtoets 3 maksimum (tacl-r) 070-999 24-26 veranderlike 11 woorddefinisie minimum (ast) 084-120 27-29 veranderlike 12 woorddefinisie maksimum (ast) 089-125 30-32 veranderlike 13 kronologiese ouderdom (mnde) 097-122 33-35 veranderlike 14 tbtale woorde (tmt) 1-9 36 veranderlike 15 woorde per τ eenheid (tmt) 1-9 37 veranderlike 16 tipe tekenratio (tmt) 1-9 38 veranderlike 17 bywoorde (tmt) 1-9 39 veranderlike 18 voorsetsels (tmt) 1-9 40 veranderlike 19 medewerkwoorde (tmt) 1-9 41 veranderlike 20 weglatings (tmt) 1-9 42 veranderlike 21 vervangings (tmt) 1-9 43 veranderlike 22 verplasings (tmt) 1-9 44 veranderlike 23 onvoltooid (tmt) 1-9 45 veranderlike 24 sinstruktuurverbetering (tmt) 1-9 46 veranderlike 25 woordkeuseverbetering (tmt) 1-9 47 veranderlike 26 woordherhaling (tmt) 1-9 48 veranderlike 27 benoemings (tmt) 1-9 49 veranderlike 28 onsin (tmt) 1-9 50 veranderlike 29 abstrak/konkreet (tmt) 1-9 51 veranderlike 30 creaghead pragmatiek vraelys 00-25 52-53 veranderlike 31 analise 1 minimum (pendulum) 076-152 64-56 veranderlike 32 analise 1 maksimum (pendulum) 079-155 57-59 veranderlike 33 sintese 1 (pendulum) 080-152 60-62 veranderlike 34 sintese 2 (pendulum) 083-155 63-65 veranderlike 35 storiegeheue 1 (pendulum) 084-152 66-68 veranderlike 36 storiegeheue 2 (pendulum) 087-155 69-71 veranderlike 37 opeenvolgende geheue 1 (pendulum) 076-116 72-74 veranderlike 38 opeenvolgende geheue 2 (pendulum) 079-119 75-77 , veranderlike 39 respondentnommer 1-9 01 veranderlike 40 kaartnommer 2 02 veranderlike 41 sluiting 1 000-132 . 03-05 ί veranderlike 42 sluiting 2 072-135 06-08 1 veranderlike 43 1-minuut spoedtoets (tod) 01-16 09-10 veranderlike 44 spelling (ipv spellingtoets) 01-16 11-12 | veranderlike 45 skriftelike taal (k3 skolastiese toets) 01-16 13-14 ; veranderlike 46 prosalees en begrip (grove en hauptfleisch) 01-16 15-16 veranderlike 47 stillees en begrip (tod taaltoets) 01-16 17-18 veranderlike 48 verbaal (ik toets) 078-114 19-21 veranderlike 49 nie-verbaal (ik toets) 086-128 22-24 veranderlike 50 totaal (ik toets) 087-121 25-27 veranderlike 51 fonologiese prosessering subtoets fonologiese 00-10 28-29 . • veranderlike 52 prosessering subtoets 2 fonologiese prosessering 00-19 30-31 veranderlike 53 subtoets 3 0-5 / 3 2 * alle subtoetse en toetse waarna verwys word, is onderskeidelik in tabelle 2 en 3 volledig bespreek en uiteengesit. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 7 the effect of guessing on the speech reception thresholds of children asoka moodley, b.ed (hons) med (audiol.) f.c.p. county service for the sensory impaired audiology education department, lincolnshire county council lincolnshire, united kingdom abstract speech audiometry is an essential part of the assessment of hearing impaired children and it is now widely used throughout the united kingdom. although instructions are universally agreed upon as an important aspect in the administration of any form ofaudiometric testing, there has been little, if any, research towards evaluating the influence which instructions that are given to a listener have on the speech reception threshold obtained. this study attempts to evaluate what effect guessing has on the speech reception threshold of children. a sample of 30 secondary school pupils between 16 and 18 years of age with normal hearing was used in the study. it is argued that the type of instruction normally used for speech reception threshold in audiometric testing may not provide a sufficient amount of control for guessing and the implications of this, using data obtained in the study, are examined. opsomming spraakoudiometrie is 'n wesenlike aspek van die totale evaluering van gehoorgestremde kinders en word tans uitgebreid in die verenigde koninkryk aangewend. daar word universeel saamgestem dat instruksies van wesenlike belang is in die uitvoering van alle vorme van oudiometriese toetsing. beperkte navorsing is egter tot op hede uitgevoer om die invloed van die instruksies, wat aan die luisteraar verskaf word op die spraakontvangsdrempel te peil. hierdie studie poog om die uitwerking van raai op die spraakontvangsdrempel van kinders te evalueer. 'n steekproefvan 30 hoerskoolleerlinge tussen die ouderdomme van 16 tot 18jaar, met normale gehoor, is in die studie benut. daar word beweer dat die tipe instruksies wat standaard gebruik word in die bepaling van die spraakontvangsdrempel, nie voldoende beheer uitoefen oor raai nie. die implikasies daarvan word ondersoek met behulp van die data wat in die studie verkry is. in performing speech audiometry it is common to obtain two measures of speech performance: the speech reception threshold (srt) and the speech ^discrimination score. the srt is .similar to the hearing threshold level for pure tones, because it is the lowest level at which the individual can just hear and repeat 50% of the test words. the discrimination score determines the person's ability to understand speech by measuring his capacity to distinguish the various phonemes of oral language at comfortable levels of loudness. ι in general the speech reception threshold and the pure tone average of the speech frequencies serve as a reliability check on each other, and should correspond closely. it has been suggested (carhart & porter, 1971) that averaging the pure tone thresholds at 500hz and lkhz, minus 2db, adequately predicts the srt from most audiometric configurations. in clinical practice the audiologist considers that agreement between speech reception threshold and pta of ± 6db is within acceptable limits (oslen & matkin, 1979). when one measure deviates substantially from the other, it can be an indication that results are suspect, and that both pure tone and speech audiometry should be repeated. within the last thirty years, speech reception threshold (srt) testing has come to play a prominent role in audiometry, makdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykins, vol. 37 1990 ing it one of the major audiometric tests available for the assessment of hearing. consistent with the magnitude of importance attached to the srt has been the great amount of research concerning numerous procedural variables that have been shown to influence the magnitude of srt obtained for a given subject. these include: 1. the form of the verbal stimuli used for the measurement of srt (harris, 1965). 2. the degree of sophistication a listener possesses with regard to vocabulary (farrimond, 1962). 3. the degree of familiarity the listener has with the test stimuli (oslen & matkin, 1979, wilson & margolis, 1983). 4. the use of live voice versus recorded presentation (beattie, forrester & ruby, 1977). although instructions are universally agreed upon as an important aspect in the administration of any form of audiometric test, little, if any, research has been directed towards evaluating the influence which the instructions that are given to a listener have on the speech reception threshold obtained. this study was concerned mainly with the possible influence that the factor of guessing, which is encouraged in essentially all suggested instructions for the speech reception threshold sash a 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 8 asoka moodley (chalklin & ventry, 1964; harris, 1965; martin, 1975; newby, 1972), may have on the resulting threshold for speech. this interest was fostered by the writer's observation that although all subjects are given similar instructions for the srt, which include encouragement to guess on items of which they are uncertain, considerable variability seems to exist in the degree of guessing which actually takes place among individuals. given this situation, it seems feasible to assume that those persons inclined to guess in an "aggressive" fashion, might obtain srt decidedly different from the srt obtained from those who choose not to engage in any guessing. the aim of this study was therefore to attempt to quantify the extent, if any, to which the factor of guessing may conceivably influence the magnitude of the srt. subjects subjects for the study included 30 secondary school pupils, made up of 15 boys and 15 girls. the average age of the boys was 16.9 years and the average age of the girls was 17.2 years. the average age of the group as a whole was 17.5 years. the age range for the group was 16.2 years 18.5 years. more details of the sample are shown in table 1. to qualify as a subject each pupil had to be: 1. between 16 and 18.5 years of age. 2. not have any prior knowledge of the srt procedure. 3. possess air conduction pure tone thresholds of 20db hl, or better, in the test ear at the octave frequencies from 250-4000hz. table 1: number, age and sex of subjects taking part in study age in years sex no. mean range boys 15 16.9 16.3 18.4 girls 15 17.2 16.2 18.5 boys & girls 30 17.5 16.2 18.5 method each pupil was seen initially to determine whether or not he or she qualified for the study. information concerning age and familiarity with the srt was ascertained initially. pupils meeting these two qualifications were then given both pure tone and srt tests in one ear randomly selected for examination. air conduction pure tone thresholds were obtained in the test ear at octave frequencies from 250-4000hz. after the completion of pure tone testing, srts were obtained in the same test ear. each pupil was administered the srt procedure under two separate conditions. in condition 1 the srt was obtained with the subjects being instructed n o t to guess on any items of which they were unsure. for condition 2 subjects were required to make a response for every spondee word presented. these two conditions were counterbalanced among subjects to control for any possible learning effects. before formal testing began each subject was familiarised with the test item by having the individual read each of the 35 spondee words. instructions were then given to each subject based on the instructions offered by (hopkinson, 1972). minor modifications in these instructions were made to reflect the differences in the test procedures to be used with each of the two test conditions. the instructions for condition 1 are given below. "words will be presented to you that have two parts: words like baseball, mushroom, and eardrum. the words will get softer and softer. just repeat each word after you have heard it. repeat each word you feel you have heard correctly. do not guess if you feel unsure of a word. are there any questions?" the same instructions were used for condition 2 with the appropriate modifications: "words will be presented to you that have two parts: words like baseball, mushroom, and eardrum. the words will get softer and softer. just repeat each word after you have heard it. it is important that you make an attempt at every word, even those of which you are uncertain. please respond on every test item, even if it is a guess. are there any questions?" test materials and procedure speech reception thresholds were obtained using commercially available tapes of the central institute for the deaf (cid) auditory test w / l . this consists of a list of 36 spondaic words, such as daybreak, birthday. six lists of w / l are available, each list being a different scrabbling of the same 36 words. the w / l recording attenuates the spondee lodb relative to the carrier phrase "say the word". this means that the carrier phrase has been recorded approximately lodb above the average level of the words. all pure tone and speech audiometry was conducted in a well carpeted room, measuring 15 χ 18 feet, free from extraneous noise and where the recorded ambient noise was no more than 40dba at various intervals during the course of the testing. all signals were generated by a kamplex τ a 155 diagnostic audiometer and transduced to each subject via tdh-39 earphones mounted in mx41/ar cushions. procedures described by (martin & pennington, 1971) with a 5db increment were utilized for establishing each srt. j the srt for each subject was obtained using a descending! method, rather than an ascending method. speech signals were presented at a comfortable level above threshold to fami j liarise the listener with the words and the task. the level was | then decreased in 5db steps, presenting one to three words at i each level decrement until a word was missed. at this point the ' stimulus level was raised lodb and then lowered in 5db steps, i presenting 3-6 spondees with each change in stimulus level. ' the level at which 50% of the spondees were correctly repeated was recorded as their speech reception threshold (srt). results the mean piire tone results obtained for the group of 30 subjects included in the study as well as the pure tone averages are presented in table 2. / mean pure tone values obtained as a function-o'f sex revealed some difference in auditory acuity between male and female subjects; the thresholds generally were better for female subjects for all frequencies tested, with the greatest difference between male and female subjects occurring at 2000hz and / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the effect of guessing on the speech reception thresholds of children 4000hz. these findings are in good agreement with results obtained by an investigation by corso (1968), where statistically significant differences in threshold values were found between male and females, which are similar to those found in this study. the mean srts obtained under conditions 1 and 2 are presented in table 3. these results are in good agreement with the pure tone data, with females yielding slightly better srts under both test conditions. an overall threshold difference in srt of 8.40 was observed between conditions 1 and 2, with condition 2 yielding the best srts. similar difference values were observed for the male and female subjects under these two test conditions. chalklin & ventry, 1964; siegenthaler & smith, 1961) has generally concluded that variables such as practice in hearing spondees at or near threshold, live voice versus recorded test stimuli, and use of two or five db increment in the measurement of the srt, have no strong clinical implications, since these variables were found to influence the srt by magnitudes of only l-2db. however, other research by (tillman & jerger, 1959) on familiarization with the test stimuli, has demonstrated that when a factor influences the srt by an amount greater than this, it then becomes clinically relevant. therefore, the implication is that the 8.40db difference found to be associated with guessing in the present study, makes it important that this variable be controlled in some fashion during the administration of the srt. the srt data obtained under conditions 1 and 2 were subject to a two-way analysis of variance procedure (winer, 1971). this was done to determine whether any statistically significant differences in srt existed between these two conditions as a function of sex and type of instructions, or as a result of any interaction between these variables. the analysis indicated a significant f value for sex (p < 0.05) and type of instructions for the srt (p < 0.01), while no significant interaction was observed between these two variables. the significant f value for the factor of sex is to be expected, due to the superior auditory acuity possessed by the female subjects in this investigation. the strong significance of the f ratio for the type of instructions clearly indicates that the factor of guessing will influence the srt obtained by a magnitude which is statistically and clinically significant. discussion the results of this study may be of relevance in the determination of the frt in a clinical environment, and may also be of use to teachers of the hearing impaired in their routine audiological assessment of hearing impaired children. previous research dealing with other variables associated with the· administration of the table 2: mean pure tone are also included the type of instruction normally used for the srt in audiometric testing may not provide a sufficient amount of control for guessing. although no formal investigation has been conducted to determine the precise extent to which individuals engage in guessing while undertaking the srt test, personal experience would suggest a considerable amount of variability in the amount of guessing that individuals actually engage in during the test. some children will guess on spondee items when they have heard little, if any, of the acoustic information in a given word, while others are prone not to engage in any guessing and will respond to the test stimuli only when they are relatively confident that they have heard the word presented. these extremes exist even though a common set of instructions, which encourage guessing, are used during testing. there seems to be too much latitude in the manner in which a given individual reacts and responds to currently used instructions for the srt: relative to the variable of guessing. this allows for two different individuals with identical hearing acuity to conceivably obtain significantly different speech reception thresholds, and also makes it potentially possible for a given person to obtain two significantly different speech reception thresholds, all because of the degree to which guessing occurs during the testing. this suggests that some modification in the instructions should be considered that will provide for more control of this impor-frt (tillman & jerger, 1959; thresholds in db hl for the 15 males and 15 females in this study. standard deviations males females combined frequency (hz) (n = 15) sd (n = 15) sd (n = 30) sd i 250 1 4.00 ± 9.1 0.66 ± 7.04 1.68 ± 8.60 500 4.00 ± 9.86 0.66 ± 7.04 1.68 ± 8.34 1000 1.34 ± 12.46 2.00 ± 9.42 0.34 ± 10.98 2000 4.00 ± 14.54 2.00 ± 12.08 1.00 ± 13.48 4000 6.66 ± 17.6 0.66 ± 12.22 3.00 ± 15.34 pure-tone average (500hz ik 2k) 3.12 ± 9.20 1.56 ± 6.40 0.78 ± 8.14 table 3: mean srt in db hl for male and female subjects in the study under condition 1 (no guessing) and condition 2 (guessing required). difference values are also given. standard deviations are provided. condition 1 sd condition 2 sd difference sd cond 1 cond 2 males (n = 15) females (n = 15) , combined (n = 30) 2.66 ± 8.12 2.80 ± 5.60 0.06 ± 7.40 6.26 ± 7.04 10.66 ± 5.22 8.46 ± 6.48 3.60 ± 1.84 7.86 ± 2.32 8.40 ± 2 . 1 2 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37: 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 asoka moodley tant factor. serious consideration should be given to adopting a set of instructions that is similar to the instructions given to the subjects in this study during condition 1 or 2. listeners should be presented with a situation where they have a very clear and definite responses pattern specified. for example, if an individual is instructed not to guess at any time and to respond only when he/she is absolutely sure of a given test stimulus, guessing will be essentially controlled. no practical means of totally controlling guessing with absolute certainty is available, but this does not seem to be a serious limitation. another important reason for the control of the guessing variable relates to the agreement observed between the speech reception threshold (srt) and the pure tone audiogram (pta). numerous researchers (jerger, et al. 1959; tillman & jerger, 1959; carhart & porter, 1971) have investigated the agreement between these two indexes and have found it to be very close for listeners in which valid audiometric data have been obtained. generally the srt should be within ± 5db of the pta for a given ear. because of the stability of the agreement between the srt and the pta, it has been used as an important index of the validity of the data obtained on each of these two tests. in this study the ptas of the 30 subjects agreed more closely with the srts obtained under condition 1, where no guessing was allowed. this comparison resulted in a mean pta of 0.78 db hl and a mean srt of 0.06db hl. if better agreement between these two indexes is desired, the results of this study suggest that consideration should be given to the elimination of guessing during the attainment of the speech reception threshold (srt). references beattie, r., forrester, p., & ruby, b. reliability of the tillman-oslen procedure for determination of spondee threshold using recorded and live voice presentations.jourwaz ofthe american audiological society, no. 2, 159-162, 1977. carhart, r. & porter, l.f. audiometric configuration and prediction of threshold for spondees./o«ra«/ of speech and hearing research, 14, 486-495, 1971. chalklin, j. & ventry, i. spondee threshold measurement: a comparison of 2 and 5db methods. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 27, 47-59, 1964. corso, j. proposed laboratory standard of normal hearing. journal of the acoustical society of america, 30, 14-23, 1958. farrimond, t. factors influencing auditory perception of pure tones and speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 5, 194204, 1962. harris, j. speech audiometry in a. glorig ed. audiometry; principles and practices. williams and wilkins, baltimore, m.d., 1965. hopkinson, n. speech reception threshold inj. j£at£-ed. handbook of clinical audiology, williams & wilkins, baltimore, m.d., 1972. jerger, j., carhart, r., tillman, t., & peterson, j. some relations between normal hearing for pure tones and for speech .journal of speech and hearing research, 2, 126-140, 1959. martin, f.n., & pennington, c.d. current trends in audiometric practices. a.s.h.a. 13, 671-677, 1971. newby, h. audiology, prentice hall, 1972. oslen, w.o., & matkin, n.d. speech audiometry. in rintleman, w.f. (ed.) hearing assessment. baltimore: university park press, 1979. siegenthaler, b., & smith, d. speech reception^thresholds by different methods of test administration .journal of the acoustical society of america, 33, 1802, 1961. tillman, t., & jerger, j. some factors affecting the spondee thresholds in normal hearing subjects. journal of speech and hearing research, 2, 141-146, 1959. wilson, r.h., & margolis, r.h. measurements of auditory thresholds for speech stimuli, principles of speech audiometry, konkle, d.f. & rintelmann, w.f. (ed), page 79-126, 1983. winer,b. statistical principles on experimental design, mcfiraw hill. new york, 1971. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language of the english-speaking coloured child rosanna m1lste1n, β .a. (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) 105-21 66 ave., forest hills, ν. y. 11375 summary the language deficient language different controversy in the language of the coloured child was investigated while at the same time examining coloured english. the ss comprised 20 nine-year old english speaking coloured children, 9 boys and 11 girls. language samples were obtained through direct questioning according to halliday's language functions. from these samples, the non-standard language structures used by a significant number of the ss were isolated, and examined. it was established that the ss had competence for all these structures except that of relative sentences. possible reasons for this were postulated. a sub-aim of the study was to comparc the ratings of coloured teachers and a group of speech therapists and university students of non-standard sentences used by the ss. based on the findings, several implications for speech therapy were discussed. opsomming die twispunt „taalafwyking" teenoor ,,taalverskil" in die taal van die kleurlingkind is ondersoek, asook terselfdertyd kleurlingengels. die proefpersone was twin tig nege jarige engelssprekende kleurlingkinders, 9 seuns en 11 dogters. taalmonsters is verkry deur direkte vrae volgens halliday se taalfunksies. daardie taalstrukture wat nie standaard is nie en wat deur 'n betekenisvolle aantal proefpersone gebruik is, is van hierdie monsters geskei en noukeurig ontleed. daar is vasgestel dat die proefpersone vaardigheid in al hierdie strukture,'behalwe in relatiefsinne, getoon het. moontlike redes hiervoor word gepostuleer. 'n sekondere doel van hierdie studie was om die beoordelings van kleurlingonderwysers teenoor die van 'n groep spraakterapeute en universiteitstudente te vergelyk met betrekking tot daardie sinne wat deur die proefpersone gebruik is en nie standaard is nie. die implikasies wat die bevindinge vir spraakterapie inhou, word bespreek. south africa is both a multi-racial and a multi-lingual society. any profession concerned with speech and language must take cognizance of this and must include all the subcultures into its sphere of interest and investigation. this will ensure better understanding, communication and interaction among the members of society. the coloured people may be viewed as one such subculture, and thus warrant investigation as have done the negroes and puerto ricans. from the point of view of the speech therapist and linguist, the study of such cultures and subcultures offers a wealth of linguistic data which can serve to improve the efficiency of the disciplines. in studying the english of coloured children, it is important to bear in mind the standard version of the language, i.e., standard english (se). speakers whose dialects vary from the standard version have been labelled either as being defective or as being linguistically different.2 this conflict stems from the tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 rosanna milstei language deficient language difference controversy. according to the language deficient, viewpoint, the speakers are seen as using an inferior language, a "restricted code" 5 ' 6 together with its concomitants of concreteness, depressed verbal performance, and low conceptualization. 5 ' 6 ' 1 4 ' 2 8 ' 2 9 ' 3 5 ' 3 8 the proponents of the language difference standpoint suggest that the language is highly organized, rule governed and systematic, and is in no way inferior.1'9'42 the differences that occur are merely in the surface structure.26 in this study, when discussing the language used by the coloured child, the term non-standard will be alternated with "error", both of which will imply avariation, rather than a mistake. from the above it can be seen that in testing, it is important to tap not only what is overt viz. performance, but also what is known and understood about the particular linguistic code viz. competence. this is important as it will determine to an extent the severity of the "error", the approach in therapy, and if in fact, there is to be therapy at all. competence cannot be directly observed and is thus difficult to assess.3'41 linguistic competence is of little use if the speaker is unable to use his language. this suggests the value of assessing communicative competence7 and the change in language as it is used to fulfill different functions. halliday 1 5 ' 1 6 discusses several such functions of which three will be investigated in this study. in the representational or textual function, language is used to express propositions about something. this imaginative or ideational model serves to relate the child to his environment in his own way. through the interactional or interpersonal model, the language is used to establish and maintain social relations.15'16 these categories may be theoretical but if they do exist, they are ripe for exploration and may have important implications for the linguist and speech therapist. in particular, they indicate the importance of taking into account the context and meaning when evaluating language. methodology aims the main aim of this study is to investigate certain aspects of the language of the english-speaking coloured child. this is done with a view to examining the language deficient language different controversy. implicit in this aim are several sub-aims: to examine the possible change in language as it is used in different language functions. the language functions to be examined are those discussed by halliday1 5'1 6 and are a) representational b) imaginative and c) interactional. 1 to investigate the possible "concreteness" in the language of the coloured child. this area is closely allied to the language deficiency hypothesis. to differentiate the "errors" made by the group as a whole as,opposed to those made idiosyncratically by individual children. these may then be characteristics of coloured english. to evaluate the in-depth testing of language using both expressive language and structured language tests. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language of the english-speaking coloured child 15 to compare coloured speakers' ratings of the children's language with those of a combined speech therapist and university population group. it is important to determine whose judgement to take into account when labelling a child's language "incorrect". to compare "errors" found with those reported in the literature on other english-speaking minority groups e.g. puerto ricans, negroes. hypothesis the language of the english-speaking coloured child is riot inferior to se but is an organised, rule-ordered language. the child is able to express and understand meaning in his environment and is thus not seen as language impaired. subjects 20 ss were used, 9 boys and 11 girls. the ss were drawn from two english medium coloured schools in johannesburg and were all in std.2. the mean age was 9 years 4 months and the ages ranged from 8 years 5 months to 9 years 8 months. the ss were selected by the teachers according to several criteria. 1. they had to be approximately between 8 years 6 months and 9 years 6 months. by this age children have acquired all the basic sentence structures and have not yet been influenced by the jargon of the peer group.27 2. they had to be of average intelligence and must not have failed at school. the experimenter (e) did not want the factor of intelligence to interfere but at the same time did riot want a biased population. for the purposes of this study, scholastic achievement was felt to be an adequate criterion. 3. the ss had to have no speech, hearing or language problem. the ε did not want their performance to be masked by any of the above. 4. the ss had to come from english-speaking homes and although variables of parental education and socio-economic status could not be controlled for, the above criteria made for a sufficiently homogeneous group. tests used these were designed in two parts: 1. the elicitation of language samples within representational, imaginative and interactional functions of language. 2. tests of those areas in which the ss had used non-standard variations of the language. no standardised tests were used as these were not felt to be appropriate for the sample being tested. the use of standardised tests has led to confusion regarding accurate descriptions of the different language of the culturally different child.40 tydskrifvan die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelknde vol 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 rosanna milstei test i a series of questions was used. these were similar to those used by lawton.28 there were three categories of questions, corresponding to the three language functions being tested'. toys were used to elicit the language which included a goblin, snake, sequence cards, jigsaw puzzles and two telephones. the first questions tapped the representational function of language. questions such as the s's name, age and address were started with, progressing to a more abstract level e.g. 'why should people not steal?' in the imaginative functions, the ss were required to talk about the rubber goblin and snake. they also had to answer the questions 'what would you do if you had a space ship?' or 'what you do if you were a fairy?', depending on their sex. for the interactional function, another s was called in and the two ss had to have a telephone conversation. they were also given a puzzle which they had to complete together. the questions were designed to elicit equal amounts of language for each of the three categories. this type of directed, structured language eliciting was felt to be better suited to the population being tested than some of the conventional methods u s e d . 6 ' 2 9 ' 3 0 ' 4 4 test ii this was designed to assess the competence in those areas of test i in which the ss had used different versions of the language. performance may be affected by certain variables e.g. memory factors, and the interference of physical or psychological processes.32 thus competence cannot be inferred from performance but must be separately investigated. only those areas which were "failed" by a significant number of the group were tested. eight areas were examined and the materials used were sentences, pictures on 17 χ 10 cm cards a boy, dog, car, train and lion. in each subtest, there were five items except for subtest 8, which had seven items. subtest 1 passives: the ss had to point to a picture corresponding to a passive sentence.23 there was a choice of four pictures so as to reduce the chance ^ factor. ss were then given an explanation of the passive, focusing on the object, and had to construct two passive sentences from the two action situations e.g. "the car hits the train" this is demonstrated "starting with the words 'the train' say what happened to the train". in these, the objects used were such as to yield no semantic cues.37 thus in the above example, either the-train or the car could have been the subject. subtest 2 relative sentences: the ss had to answer questions about relative sentences e.g. "the girl the boy saw was tall". "who was tall?" two types of embedded sentences were used: a) one embedded sentence in the underlying p-marker e.g. the cat that caught the mouse had a long tail b) one embedded sentence in the underlying p-marker but in the surface structure it is centreembedded e.g. the children the fish saw swam quickly.12 the sentences used were controlled for syllable length and here too, as far as possible yielded no semantic cues.37 journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language of the english-speaking coloured child 17 subtest 3 prepositions: the ss had to say whether the sentences were correct or incorrect, this method being referred to as sentence identification. they also had to say whether the picture presented corresponded to a sentence they were given e.g. given a picture of a car having driven into a tree, they had to say whether the sentence "the car drove in the tree" was right or wrong. subtest 4 pronouns: the ss had to point to one out of four pictures which corresponded to a.sentence. sentence identification was also used here e.g. i'll buy me a house. subtest 5 modal:the ss had to identify sentences as well as construct questions with "can", "will" and "do". even though the test words were given, it was felt that their competence would be demonstrated if they could use them correctly in a sentence. subtest 6 incomplete sentences: sentence identification was used here. subtest 7 agreement of v with ν of np and vp: sentence identification was required here. subtest 8 tense: the ss had to identify sentences and had to listen to a short story and fill in the correct word at the end e.g. would/will. the incorrect sentences used in the tests were either contrived, based on the ss' "errors", or they were actual sentences used by the ss. administration of the tests test i each s was seen individually for about 30 minutes and these interviews were tape recorded. the interview commenced with the representational function and although questions such as name, age and address yielded language data, they also served to relax the ss. the questions were asked in the same order for all ss. the imaginative function was tapped next, followed by the interactional function. here the responses were both tape recorded and written by the ε which served as a double check. test ii the second session took place one month later and the ss were seen individually for about 30 minutes. they were told they were going to play different sorts of speech games. the subtests were presented in the same order for all ss and the instructions were given at the start of each subtest. analysis and statistical procedures test i the language samples were transcribed from the tape recorder and a list was compiled of all the "errors" made by the ss. context was at all times referred to, following lee and canter.30 from this list, a table was drawn up listing the language classes and the number of "errors" made in each class by each s in each of the three categories. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheekunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 rosanna milstein using the information from the table, a chi squared test was used to determine which "errors" were made significantly by the group as a whole as opposed to individual "errors". the same test was used to determine whether all three language functions were equally affected for the ss. the chi squared test was chosen as the data being dealt with was ordinal and appeared to have directionality.34 a further table was compiled, listing the transformations the ss should have h a d 2 0 ' 2 1 and those which were present in their language samples. the chi squared test was used to establish which transformations were omitted by a significant number of the group. test 11 the ss were given scores out of 5 for the subtests. the results were tabulated, showing the number of "errors" made by each s for each subtest. the totals were subtracted from 100 to get a score of "incorrect". the chi squared test was used to determine which of the subtests were failed by a significant number of ss. teachers' ratings a list of 40 sentences used by the ss which were judged incorrect by the e, and 10 correct control sentences were given to 20 english-speaking coloured teachers and to 20 speech therapists and university students. the sentences had to.be rated as being 'acceptable', 'unacceptable', or 'not sure', if used by a nine-year old. the ratings of the two groups were compared and the pearson product moment correlation coefficient was used to establish the relationship. results and discussion results of test 1 in analysing the language samples of the ss, it was established that certain nonstandard versions were used by the group while others were idiosyncratic. this was ascertained using the chi squared test where x 2 = 33,92; df -22; ρ = 0,05. six language classes were isolated as having contained non-standard structures in the group. using the value of ε where ε = total number of errors i.e. the extotal number of cases pected values, it was found that above 11,52 the non-standard structures were used significantly by the group as a whole. the six language classes which fell above this point were "errors" of agreement, tense, prepositions, pronouns, incomplete sentences and modals (seefigure 1.). / discussion of the six "errors" agreement this involved mainly the np-v relationship e.g. "in the morning, she brush it out". other "errors" involved the relationship between v and ν of vp and np auxiliary. this was also found by ganes 1 3 in her study of the written english of coloured teacher trainees. this has also been noted in negro dialects where there are no obligatory morphological endings.3'10 tense this structure involved the substitution of one tense for another. the present tense was used for the past, conditional, progressive and future. future journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language of the english-speaking coloured child 19 h i j k l m n o p q r s t u v w language classes key: a = agreement b = tense c = preposition d = pronouns e = incomplete sentence f = modal g = word order h = clumsy figure 1: representation of the frequency of errors per language class in descending order tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde. vol. 23. desember 1976 i = determiner q = plurals j = k = auxiliary r = participles j = k = conjunction s = interrogative reversals 1 = got/my t = "to" m = do u = adj/adv η = vocabulary v = double negative 0 = semantic over elaboration w = miscellaneous ρ = copula r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 rosanna milstein tense was also used for conditional and the past tense was substituted for the future. the main "errors" were the use of future for conditional tense e.g. in response to "what would you do if you had r100?" "i will put it in the bank". it would appear that this substitution implies an inability to move out of the concrete functioning. it seems that the only areas of operation are those that are definite and tangible viz. past, present and future. some authors have reported that in negro dialects, conditionality is represented in word order change but this was not found in the protocols of this study.3 with regard to the use of present for past this may be due to the influence of the afrikaans historical present which is also evident in dialects of s e . 1 , 2 5 prepositions there were several substitutions found in this language class but it was only those which confused semantic understanding that were considered. these "errors" would be indicative of deficient conceptual relations and thus warrant attention e.g. a frequent substitution was in/at e.g. "i live in 20 north avenue". this does not impair meaning at all. in contrast, the sentence, "she was thankful for her parents (for giving her the gift)" implies a confusion of "to" and "for" as prepositions and it is this type of factor that necessitates deeper investigation. pronouns three types of non-standard versions were found. the least aberrant was that of omission and this occurred seldom. the use of the accusative pronoun for the reflexive was predominant e.g. "i'll buy me a house." this seemed to occur only in the first person.1 a further variation which was considered within this language class was where in a sentence, either the identical np was not deleted or the pronoun was redundant e.g. "my mother, she goes to town." adler1 has referred to this as "subject expression". fasold 1 0 refers to it as pronominal apposition where a pronoun is used in apposition to the ν of np. he feels that the length of the modifying material which intervenes between the noun and the pronoun affects the acceptability — the more intervening material, the more acceptable. this may be related to the entry and re-entry of participants in a narrative.10 incomplete sentences in most cases, the first np of "it" was absent. labor24 feels that these sentences cannot be considered incorrect as the child must have understood what was said in order to give an appropriate but incomplete response. this makes clear the importance of context in evaluating the language. modals these were frequently deleted and occurred mainly in the context of "got" e.g. "see, this man got a gun". 'got' was not regarded as incorrect unless it was substituted for the main verb of the sentence e.g. got/received,./ the above structures were discussed as they were found to be used by the majority of the group and thus may be seen as characteristic of coloured english. non-standard variations were also used by individuals and although they are of interest, they cannot be discussed here. language functions it was found that the three language categories were differentially affected by the "errors". using the chi squared test, where x 2 = 5,99; df = 2; ρ = 0,05 it was found that representational language had a significant number of non-standard structures x2' = 12,08. both imaginative and interactional language were not affected by the "errors", imaginative language journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language of the english-speaking coloured child 21 having x 2 = 0,005 and interactional language x2 = 12,57. it can be seen that interactional language was not affected significantly. these results should however be viewed with reservation as although the test was designed to elicit equal amounts of language for each category, it is possible that unequal amounts were obtained, making the results less comparable. transformations that were omitted. the protocols were judged against a format of transformations passive, negatives, relative sentences, adjectives, pronouns, prepositions and adverbs. it was found that the ss did not use passives, relative sentences and adverbs significantly as a group. this was established using the chi squared test where x 2 = 3,84; df = 1; ρ = 0,05. passives and relative clauses were included in test ii but adverbs were omitted as it was felt that the area was too vast to be included in the confines of the study. results of test ii relative sentences. this subtest was failed by the group as a whole. this was ascertained through the chi squared test where x 2 = 3,84; df = 1; ρ = 0,05. there thus appears to be a defect in the competence for relative sentences. the ss did not, as suggested by strohner43 rely on syntactic information but rather on strategies. a possible explanation for this may be the age of the ss. it would be of interest to test for the evidence of this structure in an older child. passives. although these were not used spontaneously, the ss appeared to have the competence as they were able to understand and generate passive sentences. this may be due to the fact that the passive is learned late and memory may be involved in its production. in addition production lags behind comprehension and this may have been operative here too. 1 2 modals. this was well coped with, suggesting the "errors" made in test i were merely performance "errors" and were brought about by other factors e.g. phonological rules, rather than by a lack of competence in these areas. tense. here too ss coped well, suggesting that the "errors" were purely on a performance level. however it was felt that there was no semantic differentiation between e.g. should/shall. when asked the difference between these two words, some ss said there was no difference, others stating the difference by giving two sentences without going into the meaning. it is difficult to draw any conclusions from this but a possible explanation may be an inability to express subtle differences between the words, a difficulty manifested by all nine-year olds. a control group would have been useful here to evaluate whether this difficulty was culture specific or age group specific. prepositions. the "errors" seemed to be purely on a performance level and the ss appeared to have the conceptual meanings of the prepositions used incorrectly. pronouns. these were well managed indicating that the ss had the competence for these but broke down on a performance level. incomplete sentences. as mentioned earlier, these were not deficient in meaning, and the fact that the competence is there is borne out by the fact that ss were able to recognise and correct these sentences. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 rosanna milstein agreement. there appeared to be the competence for agreement between np-v and v and ν of np but the breakdown is on a performance level due to extraneous variables e.g. phonological rules. thus all errors were surface structure "errors" except for relative sentences. (see table 1.) total total x2 = 3,84 language classes correct incorrect df = 1, ρ = 5% · passives 91 9 1,26 relative sentences 59 41 5,93 prepositions 87 13 ,46 pronouns 78 22 ,1 modals 95 5 2,46 incomplete sentences 85 15 ,21 agreement 83 17 ,05 tense 69,3 30,7 1,66 table i. summary of results for competence sub-test (test ii) teacher rating. as this was designed to examine the similarity or difference of the two groups' ratings, correlations were established for each of the three ratings. using the pearson product moment correlation, correlations of ,89 ,83 and ,59 were established for the three ratings respectively. this means that there was a high level of agreement between the two groups for sentences that were clearly 'acceptable' or 'unacceptable'. however, there is some disparity between the two groups on their ratings of.sentences of which they were not sure. this may be because the group of coloured teachers were more definite about their decisions, perhaps because they were familiar with both the language used in the sample sentences and the nine-year old group. the other group found it difficult to visualize how a nine-year old speaks and were thus 'not sure' more often. discussion concreteness of language. the language of the ss and their responses to the questions were felt to have been fairly concrete and situation bound. this feature was noted in various forms. it could be seen in the ss' relating of their journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language of the english-speaking coloured child 23 experiences, the story telling and in the syntax where frequently, structures implying concrete notions were used e.g. future for conditional tense. however, even though these factors may have been evident, it is incorrect to attribute them to the language of the coloured child. the language of nine-year old se speakers has not been examined and thus no statements, other than tentative ones, can be made about the language of the coloured child. furthermore, the concreteness of language must not necessarily be pinned to the speaker but rather to the factors operative at the time of testing. most obvious here is the influence of the testing situation on the language of the ss. variables in testing situation. it is inevitable that the race of e, as well as her age and authority, had an effect on the verbal output.4 0 " . . . the power relationship in a one-to-one confrontation between adult and child are too asymmetrical."24 this may have rendered the language more formal and possibly more concrete. it has been found that the greater the difference in status between the tester and s, the more the s strives to use standard forms. 1 1 ' 2 6 ' 4 0 had a different tester been used, perhaps of the same race, maybe even the same or closer age group, the ss may have given a truer representation of coloured english, along with even more non-standard structures. in addition, the poor performance may be due to a lack of understanding of the instructions. weener40 says that the ability to process natural language is directly related to the degree to which phonetic, syntactic and semantic features of a message match those same features of the speaker's own dialect. influence of afrikaans. there is yet another possible explanation for the nonstandard structures in coloured english the influence of another language, in this instance, afrikaans. although the ss were drawn from englsih-speaking homes and schools afrikaans is the dominant language of the coloured population (one english speaker to eight afrikaans speakers33). all the ss have been in contact with afrikaans speakers, speaking either afrikaans or non-standard english. the influence of one language on another has been defined differently by various authors. 8 ' 1 7 ' 1 9 features of the one language impinge on the other, and the interference is said to be in the language performance 4 the more similar the two languages, the more interference there is.3 9 this would seem to apply to the case in point, as the english used by afrikaans speakers is closer to that used by english-speaking coloureds than a completely different language e.g. afrikaans. additional aspects. a further observation that was made based on the protocols of the ss was a frequent use of sentences beginning with 'and', 'but', 'then' and 'because'. the commencement of sentences with 'and' and 'but', also found by templin44 may be viewed as misuse of conjunctions. it is possible that the ss did not have the concepts and semantic meanings of these words and thus used them at the beginning of sentences. two factors invalidate the above statement. the first is that both these words were used correctly in the language of the ss, implying that the competence for their meanings and uses does exist. secondly, these words are frequently used by speakers of se, possibly those of a lower socio-economic status. on heuristic observation, this has •frequently been noted in the language of all races, and thus must be judged with reservation. on the other hand, the frequency of the usage of these words tydskrifvan die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde. vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 rosanna milstein may be illustrative of a deficiency31 and may.be indicative of a limited vocabulary. it may be a further manifestation of the stilted, concrete nature of language which, as mentioned above, may have been partially caused by the test situation. the frequent use of 'jus' within the sentences is common in less formal speech, but may also be indicative of loss for words'. once again, judgement here must be made with reservation. conclusions from the above, there are several conclusions and implications which can be drawn, all which aid in dealing with the coloured child, be it in the form of speech therapist, teacher or research worker. broadly speaking, it would seem that the language of the english-speaking coloured child cannot be seen as inferior or deficient. it appears to serve adequately the communicative needs of the child, allows him to express himself and meaning in his environment and can be said to be deficient no more than any one language can be said to be inferior to another. in contrast, coloured english may be viewed as a different language which is how labor24 refers to the language of lower class speakers. yet, whatever the label given, there still exists an intercultural communication problem, especially when adjudged by speakers of se. implications for speech therapy there are specific implications for both testing in therapy and for the actual administration of therapy. testing. it seems, as mentioned earlier, that the method used to tap the ss' language is appropriate to the coloured population. although no spontaneous speech was directly attained, direct probing elicited sufficient language for linguistic analysis. this method of direct questioning is felt to be beneficial in a test situation where the s is not outgoing but even so, it must be tactfully handled in order not to intimidate the child. in the language samples obtained it was frequently difficult to distinguish between what was directly elicited and what was spontaneously given by the child. this would further seem to indicate the comparability and efficacy of using a structured means for getting a language sample. in addition, the linguistic content should be adapted to the child's system, thus ensuring that he understands what is required of him.3 6 further, in evaluating responses, context must be taken into account.19 this is particularly important when dealing with a culture different from that of the therapist. the therapist can no longer take for granted meanings of words used by children under test and must actively investigate the environment. this makes clear the shortfalls of using traditional tg methods for analysing and evaluating the language of coloured children. at all times semantics must be referred to, the relations of the linguistic occurrences to the rest of the child's world, and only in this way will their language be interpreted correctly and fairly. it can also be seen that it is important to probe deeper than performance as it is very often influenced by extraneous variables. thus an attempt must be made to tap competence as well as performance. therapy. the conclusion that the language, of the coloured child is different, · as opposed to deficient, throws a different light on therapy. several suggestions journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language of the english-speaking coloured child 25 have been made regarding this issue. it has been suggested that because the language is not deficient, it should be the avenue through which the child is taught the new or target language.4 the mother tongue which is used initially by the child should be used as much as possible in teaching the new language as this will promote better understanding, more carry-over between home and therapy, and the break from home will be minimized.8 the acceptance by the therapist of the child's language is of utmost importance22 and the therapist cannot be glib in admonishing a child's use of language and proclaiming it incorrect. this may result in the child's seeing himself as illogical and incorrect, and the psychological factors involved may be permanently damaging. the child will react to his failure and what may have originally been a language problem may grow into a learning and emotional one. yet another implication for the speech therapist is that she must not only accept the language of the child but must understand it and if necessary, endeavour to use i t . 7 ' 1 8 "such use, should among other gains, foster better social acceptance of the clinician by the child."2 this is important as the therapeutic relationship plays a large role. in addition, an understan ding ot the s child's culture, his needs and values14 and the dynamics operative in therapy are expedient in the successful dealing with the child, especially if he belongs to a different subculture to that of the therapist. in actually teaching structures, the therapist must determine the cause of the "errors". if the child is lacking in competence, therapy will be geared to its training if however the "error" is on a performance level only, the child will need less instruction and his attention may merely be drawn to the standard production if the "error" is related to the factors in his knowledge e.g. phonological system, there is little reason to teach the child the structure without taking into account its determinants. the findings with regard to the differential occurrence of non-standard structures within different language functions are also of interest to the speech therapist. this may give an indication as to which language functions should be started with and which may be used to facilitate carryover. the high correlation that existed between the ratings of the children's sentences by the teachers and the group of speech therapists and university students indicates that to an extent, an incorrect structure is universally incorrect, regardless of the culture or status of the judge. for the speech therapist, it means that the opinions and judgements of the teachers can be relied on. this does not mean that no further investigation need be done but does imply that the teachers of the children are reliable referrants. in the area of coloured language, the field is wide open to investigation. studies controlling many of the independent variables may lead to the isolation of factors pertinent to and influential in the understanding, description or treatment of children. the variation of language within the group as a whole may be of interest e.g. the change of language with race, sex, status and age. it would be of interest to see if at any age, competence for relative sentences is acquired. these investigations would surely have invaluable implications for the speech tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde. vol 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 rosanna milsteiri therapist. just as it was of interest to see what the coloured teachers rated correct and incorrect, so would it be enlightening to determine what the coloured children themselves term correct or incorrect. do these evaluations differ with status in the community? above all, norms are required for age groups, sex, status, all of which may validate the work with these children or refute therapy on the grounds that the reference points have been unrealistic. references 1. adler, s. (1971): dialectal differences: professional and clinical implications./ speech hear. dis. 36(1), 90-100. 2. adler, s. (1973): social class bases of language: a re-examination of socioeconomic, sociopsychological and sociolinguistic factors. asha (1)3-9. 3. baratz, j.c. (1969): language and cognitive assessment of negro children: assumptions and research need.^s/w 11 (2) 87-92. 4. baratz, j.c. (1970): should black children learn white dialect? asha 12(9)415417. 5. bernstein, b. (1959): a public language; some sociological implications of a linguistic form .brit j. sociol. 10, 311-326. 6. bernstein, b. (1962): social class, linguistic codes and grammatical elements. lang. speech 5, 221-240. 7. cazden, c.b. (1972): child language and education. holt, rinehart and winston, inc. 8. crothers, e. and suppes, p. (1967): experiment in second-language learning. academic press inc. 9. destefant, j.s. (1973): black english. in language, society and education: a profile of black english. destefano, j.s. (ed.) charles a. jones publishing company. 10. fasold, r.w. and wolfram, w. (1973): some linguistic features of negro dialect. in language, society and education: a profile of black english. destefano, j.s. (ed.) charles a. jones publishing company. 11. fishman, j. (1964): guidelines for testing minority group children. /. soc. issues 20 (2) 129-145. 12. gaer, e.p. (1967): children's understanding and production of sentences. . /. verb. learn. verb. behav. 8, 289-294. 13. ganes, g. (1967): an analysis of errors in the written english of 28 coloured teacher-trainees. unpublished research report, department of phonetics and linguistics, university of witwatersrand, johannesburg. 14. ginsberg, h. (1972): the myth of the deprived child. prentice hall inc. 15. halliday, m.a.k. (1973): explorations in the functions of language. edward arnold ltd, london. 16. halliday, m.a.k. (1970): language structure and language function. in new horizons in linguistics, lyons, j. (ed.) penguin books ltd. 17. halliday, m.a.k. (1968): the users and uses of language. in readings in the sociology of language, fishman, j.a. (ed.). mouton and-co printers, the hague. 18. honeyford, r. (1972): class talk. brit. j. dis commun. 7(2) 206-211. 19. hymes, d. (1971): competence and performance in linguistic theory. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 19 76r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language of the english-speaking coloured child 27 in language acquisition: models and methods, huxley, r. and ingram, e. (eds.). academic press inc. london. 20. jacobs, r.a. and rosenbaum, p.s. (1967): grammar 1. ginn and company, u.s. 21. jacobs, r.a. and rosenbaum, p.s. (1967): grammar 2. ginn and company, u.s. 22. johnson, k.r. (1970): blacks. in reading for the disadvantaged: problems of linguistically different learners, horn, t.r. (ed.) harcourt, brace and world, inc. 23. kessel, f.s. (1970): the role of syntax in children's comprehension from ages 6-12. monogr. soc. res. child. developm. 35(6). 24. labor, w. (1973): the logic of nonstandard english. in language, society and education: a profile of black english, destefano, j.s. (ed.). charles a. jones publishing company, u.s. 25. labor, w. and cohen, p. (1973): some suggestions for teaching standard english to speakers of nonstandard urban dialects. in language, society and education: a profile of black english, destefano, j.s. (ed.). charles a. jones publishing company, u.s. 26. labor, w. and cohen, p. (1973): systematic relations of standard and nonstandard rules in the grammar of negro speakers. in language, society and education: a profile of black english, destefano, j.s. (ed.). charles a. jones publishing company, u.s. 27. lanham, l.w. (1975): personal communication. professor, department of phonetics and linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. 28. lawton, d. (1969): social class, language and education. routledge and kegan paul ltd. london. 29. lawton, d. (1964): social class language differences in group discussions. lang. speech 7(3) 183-204. 30. lee, l.l. and canter, s.m. (1971): development sentence scoring: a clinical procedure for estimating syntactic development in children's spontaneous speech. / speech hear. dis. 36(3) 315-340. 31. leonard, l.b. (1972): what is deviant language? j. speech hear. dis. 37(4) 427446. 32. lyons, j. (1970): chomsky. fontana modern masters, london. 33. mainwaring, k. (1962): the contribution of the coloured people to culture in south africa. in understanding one another. s.a. institute of race relations. 34. mccall, r.b. (1970): fundamental statistics for psychology. harcourt, brace and world, inc. u.s. 35. mcdavid, r.i. (jnr) (1966): dialectal differences and social differences in an urban society. in sociolinguistics, bright, w. (ed.) mouton and co. publishers, the hague. 36. patterson, s. (1953): colour and culture in south africa. routledge and kekan paul limited, london. 37. penn, c. (1972): a linguistic approach to the detection of minimal language dysfunction in aphasia. unpublished research report, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of witwatersrand, johannesburg. ' tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde. vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 rosanna milstei 38. riessman, f. (1962): the culturally deprived child. harper and brothers publishers, new york. 39. saville, m.r. (1970): language and the disadvantaged. in reading for the disadvantaged: problems of linguistically different learners, horn, t.r. (ed.) harcourt, brace and world, inc. u.s. 40. severson, r.a. and guest, k.e. (1972): toward the standardised assessment of the language of disadvantaged children. in language and poverty: perspectives of a theme, williams, f. (ed.) markham publishing company, chicago. 41. shriner, t.h. and miner, l. (1968): morphological structures in the language of disadvantaged and advantaged children. in/. speech hear. res. 11(3) 605-610. 42. shuy, r.w. (1972): the sociolinguist and urban language problems. in language and poverty: perspectives on a theme, williams, f. (ed.) markham publishing company, chicago. 43. strohner, h. and nelson, k.e. (1974): the young child's development of sentence comprehension: influence of event probability, nonverbal context, syntactic form and strategies. child developm. 45(3) 567-576. 44. templin, m.c. (1957): certain language skills in children. university of minnesota press, minneapolis. 45. (1968): the use of vernacular languages in education: the report of the unesco meeting of specialists, 1951. in readings in the sociology of language, fishman, j.a. (ed.) mouton and co. printers, the hague. books on speech and hearing johannesburg: campus bookshop 34 bertha st braamfontein 724-8541 westdene services 76 king george st hospital hill 724-9345 logan's university bookshop 229 francois road durban telephone 354111 westdene services 604 eagle bldg murchies passage west street telephone 24403 westdene services sanso centre adderley street cape town telephone 411061 • westdene services shop 35 / ground floor nedbank plaza beatrix street arcadia telephone 26336 pietermaritzburg: logan's university bookshop nedbarik plaza scottsville telephone 41580/41588/41589 literary services (pty) ltd durban: cape town: pretoria: journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23, december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) , | | \ 7 3 2 m e d i c a l c i t y i t l t l l l l l l 1 e l o f f cor. jeppe s t r e e t β · | | 1 | 1 1 ι ii ii johannesburg, transvaal r e p u b l i c 7 ' ' i i i i 11 i i i i i t e l e p h o n e 2 3 6 6 8 5 h e a r i n g a i d 1 , 1 hi -.o. .*» b2o4, c o n s u l t a n t s cpty) l t d . '· saxonwold 2132 hearing aids. we specialize in the supply and fitting of hearing aids for all hearing losses, especially for nerve deafness recruitment bone conduction cases. cross aids: cros bicros multicros etc. bone conduction aids for body, earlevel, glasses (speciality by viennatone) binaural fittings we import and stock :— viennatone, qualitone, microson, phonak hearing aids. moulds: soft, hard, skeleton, vented, occluded etc. repairs: all aids supplied with a scientific performance report after repair. special prices for dealers and institutions. accessories: teacher — pupil, parent — child, individual audiotrainers, very reasonably priced. group audio trainers. tv — wireless infrared transistor receiver sets made by sennheiser. audiometers: screening, diagnostic, research, era and electrocochleography c.o.r. and peep show. impedance bridges. manufacturers of sound proof booth and sound proof rooms. hearing aid testing set by "fonix" u.s.a. phonak noise generator with different frequencies, pure tone and warble tone, for everyday's use. we repair and calibrate audiometers. sound level meters, calibrators. industrial noise consultants. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the renfrew word finding scale : application to the south african context jenny pahl meenakumari b. kara department of speech and heanng therapy university of durhan-westville, durban abstract the renfrew word finding scale (renfrew, 1988) was administered to 30 indian (group a) and30 white (group b) durban english speaking children aged between eight and nine years to determine its suitability for assessment of expressive vocabulary. mean scores for both groups were statistically compared to the british norms in terms of mean raw scores and mean mental age. mean scores for groups a and β were compared to each other. item analyses were carried out to obtain further information regarding possible lexical characteristics for each group and common problems with certain items. both groups performed significantly poorer than expected according to the british norms. group a was significantly lower than group b, thus indicating the test's unsuitability for use with these population groups in its present form. opsomming die "renfrew word finding scale" (renfrew, 1988) is op 30 indier (groep a) en 30 blanke (groep β) engelssprekende kinders tussen die ouderdomme agt en negejaar, woonagtig in durban, getoets om hierdie toets se toepasbaarheid vir die bepaling van ekspressiewe woordeskat vas te stel. die gemiddelde tellings vir beide groupe is statistics vergelyk met die britse norme ten opsigte van gemiddelde routellings en gemiddelde verstandsouderdom. gemiddelde tellings vir group a en β is ook met mekaar vergelyk. itemanalise is uitgevoer om verdere inligting aangaande moontlike leksikale kenmerke vir elke afionderlike groep, sowel as gemeenskaplike probleme van sekere items te verkry. beide groepe het beduidend swakker as wat verwag was, ten opsig van die britse norme presteer, en groep a was beduidend laer as groep b. die resultate dui dus daarop dat die huidige toets nie toepaslik is vir hierdie populasie groepe nie. the,introduction of the new dispensation in south africa has resulted in many changes opportunity for the admission ethnic groups within the school to its society, including the and integration of different system. this integration has implications for speech therapists, who will increasingly be required to provide services to people of language and cultural backgrounds different from their own. at present the framework for this service, both theoretical and practical, is primarily euro-american in nature (taylor,/1986), and not always appropriate to the south african context. an important part of the speech therapist's provision of services is assessment, which is aimed at differentiating between those people presenting with a communication problem and those whose communication is adequate. according to malan (1981), recent sociolinguistic research concerning normative differences in the language of children has necessitated a reevaluation of the current approaches to the assessment and diagnosis of language disorders in south africa. she has stressed the importance of obtaining normative information pertaining to specific communities because the commonly used measures of linguistic behaviour are based on the norms of the mainstream language. this has great significance in differentiating between true language pathology and language difference or variation. by failing to make the distinction, a difference could be perceived as a disorder, which would constitute a misdiagnosis, and could result in inappropriate and unnecessary tljerapy (anderson, 1984). the need for appropriately norm-referenced techniques in the assessment process is therefore explicit (alant & beukes, 1986). the acknowledgement of cultural factors contributed to the differencedeficit controversy concerning language. the deficit theory holds in america, for example, that "black" english is inferior to standard or "white" english. in contrast, the difference theory (e.g., labov, 1968; baratz & povitch, 1967, cited by williams 1970) holds that all languages and dialects are equal. the use of standardised tests in the assessment process can provide useful and reliable information, but in the south african situation it is frequently fraught with problems. one of the acknowledged major limitations of standardised tests is that of test bias. adler (1979) has outlined ways in which tests can be biased towards culturally different children, including the bias of linguistic aspects, the bias of non-linguistic factors, a culture-specific verbal style being required by the test, and bias due to the test's being outside the general experience of the testee. jensen (1980) discussed test bias in terms of the content validity of test items and the standardisation population. a test user, such as a speech therapist, may subjectively evaluate the appropriateness of the test according to these aspects, and may judge the test itself as culturally biased. such subjective judgments should be investigated. in the multilingual and multicultural south african conthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 ® sasi.ha 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 jenny pahl & meenakumari b. kara text, there are obviously many problems encountered by speech therapists in assessment in general and in particular, when using standardised tests. the implications of these difficulties include the provision of inadequate or inappropriate services, especially when working with people from different cultural backgrounds. alant and beukes (1986) have stressed that the limitations of standardised tests for use with different populations should be recognised, and effort should be made to reduce or eliminate the cultural and socioeconomic biases in these tests. this can be done by renorming existing tests, adapting test items or developing local norms (see, for example the discussion about this by vaughn-cooke, 1983). an aspect of communication frequently assessed by speech therapists is language development. the content component, one of three language components proposed by bloom and lahey (1978), encompasses meaning or semantics (owens, 1988). in this paper, the focus will be on an important subcomponent of language content, vocabulary. the acquisition of vocabulary, which is an integral aspect of language development, is never complete (rees, 1980) and a person's lexicon continues to expand throughout life (owens, 1988). jensen (1980) has stated that the acquisition of word meanings is highly dependent on the context in which the words are encountered. furthermore, vocabulary is not acquired by rote memorisation or formal instruction, but through generalisation, discrimination, education and inference. the child thus acquires the vocabulary of his/her cultural group (jensen, 1980). many researchers have documented the gradual (leonard & fey, 1979) but rapid and extensive acquisition of vocabulary (john & goldstein, 1964; owens, 1988). effective communication is dependent upon the retrieval of lexical items coding this knowledge from stored memory. according to wiig and semel (1984), in attempts to recall a word from long term memory, several processes are necessary. these include the selection of concepts and relationships to match the ideas, attitudes and feelings to be expressed; and the selection of vocabulary equivalents for the targeted concepts and relationships. the speech therapist is frequently concerned with the assessment of vocabulary development in children, for which (s)he uses standardised tests. although various tests are available, few if any have been standardised for the language and dialect groups in south africa. the renfrew word finding scale (rwfs) developed by renfrew (1968) in britain is one such test, that is widely used by speech therapists in durban for assessing expressive vocabulary. such assessment is valuable in identifying language difficulties in children (cottrell, montague, farb & thorne, 1980), but if cultural influences and differences are not considered, such testing can be problematic. since the first edition of the rwfs, published in 1968, there have been two revisions with the third edition having been published in 1988 (renfrew, 1988). a survey of the literature reveals that there are no reported studies on the application of this test to different cultural and ethnic groups in south africa. from the above, it appears that the relevance of the british norms and individual test items is questionable and should be examined to ensure accurate assessment of expressive vocabulary. the purpose of this study was thus to investigate the suitability of this british test of expressive vocabulary for a group of english speaking indian and white children between eight and nine years of age in durban. methodology aims the primary aim of this study was to obtain information on the performance of english speaking indian (group a) and white (group b) children to determine the suitability of the rwfs by comparing the performance of both groups with the british norms; and by comparing the results of the two groups to identify possible cultural and ethnic group influences on test performance. the secondary aim was to carry out a qualitative and quantitative item analysis to investigate the suitability of the test items, and to explore the possibility of specific vocabulary characteristics of both groups. subjects 1. description of subjects. the subjects were 30 south african indian (group a) and 30 south african white (group b) children, 15 males and 15 females in each group. the subjects ranged in age from eight to nine years. 2. subject selection criteria. renfrew(1968; 1972; 1988) selected subjects for the standardisation of the rwfs on the basis of the criteria that all children should be between the ages of three and nine years; be from english speaking homes; and have normal speech and language abilities. the same criteria were utilised in this study with the following modifications. due to practical limitations, subjects in this study were between eight and nine years of age. this age range was chosen as it was predicted that, as this level is the highest chosen by renfrew (1988), the subjects would be able to attempt all items included in the test, thereby providing the maximum amount of information regarding the suitability of items. equal numbers of boys and girls were chosen in each group to note if there were significant differences in scores obtained due to the sex variable, as renfrew (1988) indicated a sex difference with regard to scores obtained. any subject who had failed a year at school was excluded from the study. all subjects were required to be judged by their teachers as being of normal intelligence and as having normal speech and language abilities. although renfrew (1988) did not control for the class variable, subjects for this study were chosen from the middle socioeconomic group to exclude a possible source of differences. adler (1979) has noted that values relating to class cultures are reflected in linguistic interactions between children and their families, which could lead to class differences in linguistic development. apparatus the standardised test materials of the rwfs (1988) were used. the rwfs is utilised to assess children's ahlility to use words in a picture naming task. the test consists of 45 black and white line drawings on separate cards requiring 50 naming responses, which are scored on a recording form. ι procedure | all subjects at each of the schools were tested in the same room in the respective schools with only the subject and examiner present. the rwfs was administered with the examiner seated at a table next to the subject in a single session, lasting 10-15 minutes. administration and scoring procedures as outlined in the test manual were utilised. if the child was unable to recognise a picture or a targeted part, therefore, suitable explanations were given (renfrew, 1988, p.6). a modification to this procedure was to use all test items, as opposed to beginning at the starting points (items 17 and 26) for the age groups under study, in order to achieve the aim of evaluating the suitability of test items. analysis of data 1. after administration of the test, a raw score representing the total number of correct responses, was obtained for each subject according to the scoring procedures outlined in the the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) renfrew word finding scale: application1 >to the south african context 71 rwfs manual. using informal inspection, the mean raw scores for groups a and β were compared to the norms. the raw scores were converted to derived age level scores (referred to as mental age in this study for convenience) and mean mental ages were calculated for males and females in each group. 2. to determine if there were significant differences and variations between mean scores obtained by each group and the standardised british norms due to differences in sex, ethnic group and the interaction between these two variables (k. c. ryan, personal communication, april, 1991), a two-way analysis of variance (anova) was calculated. further, to identify differences within the ethnic groups (south african indian, white, and british), the tukey test, hsd (honestly significant difference) which is a q post hoc test, was done. 3. to check for significant differences between mean mental age and mean chronological age for groups a and b, the student's t-ratio test for correlated data was used. further, a ttest was used to determine if there was a significant difference between the mean mental age scores obtained by the south african groups. 4. quantitative and qualitative item analyses were carried out on the responses of the subjects on the rwfs in an attempt to obtain information about their familiarity with the items as well as characteristics of south african indian english (saie) and south african english (sae). adler (1979, p.88) has suggested the use of this procedure with different ethnic groups to "compare a child's linguistic performance to both standard and peer group norms". this will provide information concerning the difficulty of the item (downie & heath, 1974). results 1. informal inspection of groups a and β scores in comparison to british norms. the average raw scores of both groups a and β were equivalent to the scores obtained by younger children in the standardisation sample. group a (indian) obtained scores equivalent to the 5.6 to 6.0 year old age level, which is approximately 2.6 to 3.0 years below mean ca, while group β (white) obtained scores equivalent to the 7.0 to 7.6 year old age level, which is approximately 1.6 to 2.0 years below mean ca. 2. statistical comparison of the performance of the south african groups to the british norms. these results are presented in table 1. a significant difference (p<.05) in performance on the rwfs between the south african and british populations was found due to the ethnic group variable (see table 1). the results of the post-hoc test revealed that a significant difference occurred between the three ethnic groups. the sex variable was not found to be significant (p> .05) in all ethnic groups, and the combined effects of ethnic table 1: anova for scores obtained by groups a and β compared to the standardised scores on the rwfs (1988) source ss ms df f p-value p-level ethnic group 1372.422 686.211 2 53.51 0.000 ρ < .05 sex 25.600 25.600 1 1.996 0.161 ρ > .05 ethnic group and sex 0.067 0.033 2 0.003 0.997 ρ > .05 key: ss sum of squares ms mean squares df degree of freedom f f value p-value probability values p-level probability level table 2: comparison of mean ma to mean ca scores on the rwfs for groups a and β group mca j . mma m-d sd-d df t p-value p-level a males 8.6 6.0 2.6 0.83 14 11.26 0.000 ρ < .05 females 8.7 6.1 2.6 1.10 14 7.973 0.000 ρ < .05 β males 8.6 j 7.4 1.2 1.09 14 3.345 0.005 ρ < .05 females 8.9 7.4 1.5 1.03 14 3.744 0.002 ρ < .05 key: mca mean chronological age df degrees of freedom mma mean mental age t t value m-d mean difference p-value probability values sd-d standard deviation difference p-level probability level table 3: comparison between group a and group β for mean ma scores on the rwfs m-d sd-d df t p-value p-level a to β females -1.293 1.260 14 -3.975 0.001 ρ < .05 a to β males -1.387 1.215 14 -4.419 0.001 ρ < .05 key: m-d mean difference sd-d standard deviation difference df degrees of freedom t t value p-value probability values p-level probability level the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 jenny pahl & meenakumari b. kara group and sex were not significant (p>.05). 3. statistical comparison of mean ma and mean ca scores for groups a and b. it can be seen in table 2 that there were significant differences (p<.05) between ca and ma scores for both group a and group b, as indicated by the results of the student's t-test. in group a (indian), male and female subjects scored a mean ma of 2.6 years lower (6.0 and 6.1 respectively) than their mean ca (8.6 and 8.7 respectively). in group β (white), males scored a mean ma of 1.2 years lower than their mean ca (8.6), and females scored a mean ma of 1.5 years lower than their mean ca (8.9). only six subjects in all obtained ma scores higher than their ca scores. the results of the t-test comparing the ma scores of group a to those of group β presented in table 3 indicated a significant difference (p<.05) between the groups. group a performed poorer than group β for both sexes. 4. quantitative and qualitative item analysis. for the purposes of the present study it was necessary to determine an arbitrary/subjective cut off point to categorise responses to items as appropriate or inappropriate. those items correctly named by 50% or more of the subjects were classified as appropriate and those items correctly named by less than 50% of the subjects were categorised as inappropriate. these results are presented in figure 1. subjects in group a responded appropriately to 35 items and inappropriately to 15 items while in group β subjects responded appropriately to 42 items and inappropriately to 8 items (see figure 1). items one to 25 were easily named by both groups with the exception of item 18 (suitcase) for group a (indian). items 26 to 50 reflect variable levels of difficulty for the subjects. seven items (spout, 120 %age correctly identified 1 2 3 4 5 8 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 16 18 17 18 19 80 2 1 2 2 2 3 24 2 6 rwfs items i group a i group β %age correctly identified 2 8 27 2 8 2 9 3 0 3 1 32 3 3 34 36 38 37 38 3 9 4 0 4 1 4 2 4 3 4 4 4 6 4 8 4 7 4 8 4 9 6 0 rwfs items i group a i group β figure 1: percentage of correct responses given by subjects to each item on rwfs cuff, flame, wick, sling, spire/steeple and hinge) were inappropriately responded to by both groups. discussion the performance of the two south african groups was significantly lower than the british norms. it appears that differences attributed to the different cultural and ethnic backgrounds of south african whites and indians play a significant role in the acquisition of vocabulary. this is consistent with adler's (1971) observation that the patterns of behaviour unique to specific cultural groups are derived from their ethnic heritage. it therefore appears that ethnic group membership has diverse effects on naming. the suitability of the rwfs for south african indian and white children is therefore questionable. the implication of only six subjects (10%) obtaining scores equal to or above their chronological age levels, is that all the remainder have expressive vocabularies not commensurate with ca. another measure of selecting children requiring therapy because of poor expressive vocabularies, is to consider those whose test performance falls below the 25th percentile, or below the middle fifty percent range (renfrew, 1988). in the present study, 26 subjects from group a, and 13 subjects from group β could, using the british norms, be diagnosed as having expressive vocabulary problems and could require therapy. all subjects however were judged by their teachers as having normal language abilities, although the subjects were not tested specifically for language problems. the performance of the subjects scoring below the 25th percentile on the rwfs therefore should be viewed as either different or defective. depending on the theoretical perspective of individual speech clinicians, either view can be adopted. if the deficit theory is adopted, the presence of a language disorder is suggested which would require intervention. thus, according to manickam (1985, p.35) "although the individual may be functioning adequately in his [sic] socio-cultural environment, he [sic] is still viewed as being deficient with regard to the norms and is considered a candidate for an intervention programme". on this basis of incorrectly identifying children for therapy, the test itself does not appear to be a suitable assessment procedure. in contrast, results of this study seem to support the difference theory which indicates that because the norms for the rwfs were not established for the population groups investigated, the test norms are inappropriate for these groups. furthermore, as stated by adler (1979, p.95), and pertaining to this particular test, "unless a particular test has been standardised on a given group of culturally different people, it is to be expected that the scores of the culturally different will be lower than normal". it seems that because this test wjas standardised on the british population, and because test items were selected with that population in mind, subjective judge; ments of its being biased and unfair when used on other population groups would be correct. the rwfs does not appear to be sensitive to dialectal and cultural differences of south african indian and white groups. the need for investigation before making subjective judgements about test suitability is however vital. although the effect of the sex variable was not found to be statistically significant, the mean scores of .males in both groups was one point higher than the mean scores for females. such a difference is consistent with renfrew's (1990) scores and with her suggestion that a number of items are more familiar to boys than girls (e.g.tools). a further aim of the study was to investigate the relevance of specific test items. difficulty with apparently unclear drawings was noted with "diver" "lighthouse" "hinge" "sling" the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) renfrew word finding scale: application to the south african context 73 "thermometer" and "pod" (items 40,28,50,47,45, and 44 respectively). as the test is not one of picture recognition, perhaps these items should be more clearly represented in pictures. frequent incorrect responses were noted for some items, for example, "nail" for "screw" and "bandage" for "sling". on some items, cultural differences appeared to influence naming responses. the item "spire/steeple" (see item 48 in figure 1) was correctly named by nearly half of the white children, but was not correctly named by any children in the indian group. this lexical item thus seems to present a problem to both groups, but especially to the latter group. a lexical item used consistently differently by the two groups of subjects was "suitcase" (see item 18 in figure 1). in group a, 87% of subjects incorrectly identified the picture as "bag" whereas in group b, 97% of subjects correctly identified it as "suitcase" or "case". this would seem to indicate a lexical difference between south african indian english and south african english speakers. in testing, the speech therapist should thus be careful not to assume that target lexical items should be identical among all english speakers. overall, the test does not appear to be suitable for use with south african indian or white children between eight to nine years of age. for the indian group, scores were so different from the british standardisation sample that it does not seem that the test could be used to differentiate between language difficulties and normal language. for the white group, however, the test could possibly be modified for use. one means of accomplishing this, is to reorder items in terms of level of difficulty as was found to be necessary for the new zealand edition (c. renfrew, personal communication, may, 1991). an overview of the items as represented in figure 1 suggests that reordering item numbers 30, 27, 43, and 31 ("cuff' "spout" "wick" and "drill" respectively) towards the end of the test might be a solution. conclusion the present study was aimed at investigating the suitability of the rwfs for south african indian and white children ages ranging from eight to nine years. this aim was achieved by comparing the performance of the south african children to the british standardisation population. results revealed significant differences in mean scores between populations due to ethnic group membership as seen on the anova and the ttest. furthermore, comparisons of mean mas between group a and group β also revealed a significant difference which could be indicative of cultural differences between these groups, as confirmed by an examination of the responses on the item analysis. in its present form the rwfs is not suitable for use as a standardised language tool for assessing expressive vocabulary by south african speech clinicians for the populations investigated. the test could possibly be modified by reordering the sequence of items for the white group, but such modifications do not seem possible for the indian group. acknowledgements sincere appreciation is extended to the directors of education in the house of delegates and natal education departments for granting permission to conduct the fieldwork in the schools concerned. references adler, s. (1971). dialectal differences : professional clinical implications. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 36, 90-95. adler, s. (1979). poverty children and their language : implications for teaching and treating. new york : grune and stratton. alant, e. & beukes, s. m. (1986). the application of the revised version of the peabody picture vocabulary test (ppvt-r) to non-mainstream children. the south african journal of communication disorders, 33, 8-9. anderson, j. (1981). considerations in phonological assessment. in j. erickson & d. omark (eds.), communication assessment of the bilingual bicultural child. baltimore : university park press. bloom, l. & lahey, μ. (1978). language development and language disorders. new york : john wiley & sons. cottrell, a. w., montague, j., farb, j. & thorne, j. m. (1980). an operant procedure for improving vocabulary definition performances in developmentally delayed children .journal of speech and hearing disorders, 55, 90-102. downie, ν. m. & heath,.r. w. (eds.). (1974). basic statistical methods. new york : harper and row. jensen, a. r. (1980). bias in mental testing. london : methuen. john, v. p. & goldstein, l. s. (1964). the social context of language acquisition. merrill-palmer z>uarterly, 10, 265-275. leonard, l. b. & fey, μ. e. (1979). the early lexicons of normal and langauge disordered children : developmental and training considerations. in n. j. lass (ed.), speech and language : advances in basic research and practice vol 2. new york : academic press. malan, k. c. (1981). an investigation of non-standard english syntax in 12 year old coloured children. the south african journal ofcommunication disorders, 28, 68-80. manickam, l. (1985). an investigation into the performance of normal english speaking south african indian adults on the peabody picture vocabulary test (dunn, 1965). unpublished b. speech and hearing therapy research project, university of durban-westville. owens, r. e. (1988). language development :an introduction. (2nd ed.). columbus : charles e. merrill. rees, n. s. (1980). learning to talk and understand. in t. s. hixon, l. d. shriberg, & j. h. saxman (eds.), introduction to communication disorders. englewood cliffs, n.j. : prentice hall. renfrew, c. e. (1972). word finding vocabulary scale. (2nd ed.). oxford : c. e. renfrew. renfrew, c. e. (1988). word finding vocabulary scale. (3rd ed.). oxford : c. e. renfrew. renfrew, c. e. word finding vocabulary scale. (new zealand edition). oxford : c. e. renfrew. taylor, o. (1986). treatment of communication disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse populations. (ed.). california : collegehill. vaughn-cooke, f. b. (1983). improving language assessment in minority children. asha,25, 29-33. wiig, ε. h. &semel, ε. μ. (1984). language assessment and intervention for the learning disabled. london : charles e. merrill. williams, f. (ed.). (1970). language and poverty. chicago : markham. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spastic dysphonia: a case r e p o r t lesley wolk, b . a . ( s p & η t h e r a p y ) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary an in-depth investigation of a 55-year-old woman with spastic dysphonia was performed. the findings from otological, neurological and psychological investigations assisted in making a differential diagnosis and served to provide evidence for the etiology of this disorder. subjective-perceptual evaluations of the voice revealed a strained, harshbreathy voice quality with frequent breaks in phonation, a variable pitch and visible tension in the face and neck muscles. objective spectrographic evaluations revealed much turbulence, ill-defined harmonics, a breakdown in formant structure, rapid pitch fluctuations and evidence of diplophonia, which was confirmed on a fiberscopic examination. post-treatment spectrographic evaluations indicated an improved phonatory ability with significant improvement in the above parameters. results are discussed in terms of the etiology and symptomatology of this disorder; and clinical implications for diagnosis and treatment are considered. opsomming 'n 55-jarige vrou met spastiese disfonie is in diepte ondersoek. die bevindings van otologiese, neurologiese en sielkundige ondersoeke het bygedra tot 'n differensiele diagnose en het ook gedien om bewyse te verskaf vir die etiologie van hierdie afwyking. 'n gestremde grof-ruiserige stemkwaliteit met frekwente onderbrekings in fonasie, veranderlike toonhoogte en sigbare stremming in gesigen nekspiere blyk uit subjektiefperseptuele evaluasie van die stem. objektiewe spektografiese evaluasies bring baie omstuimigheid, vaag bepaalde harmoniese tone, 'n afbraak in formant struktuur, vinnige wisseling van toonhoogte en bewyse van diplofonie aan die lig. dit is bevestig deur 'n fiberskopiese ondersoek. 'n verbeterde fonetoriese vermoe met opmerklike vooruitgang in die bogenoemde parameters blyk uit spektografiese evaluasies wat na behandeling opgeneem is. resultate word bespreek in die lig van die etiologie en simptomatologie van hierdie afwyking en kliniese implikasies vir diagnose en behandeling word oorweeg. spastic dysphonia has long been recognized as the most mysterious and the most poorly understood of all voice disorders. it is a rare disorder and literature on the subject is limited, which has led to some confusion with regard to differential diagnosis and treatment. spastic dysphonia was first described by traube in 1871.6 since then many terms have been used to describe the disorder. aronson, brown, litin and pearson, 1 for example, prefer the term 'spasmodic' dysphonia to avoid the confusion that arises from the use of the term 'spastic', which implies a specific neurological disorder. it is often referred to as 'stammering of the vocal cords' or 'laryngeal stuttering' because of the similarity between the glottal symptoms of spastic dysphonia and the oral ones of the stutterer. for the. purpose of this paper, the term 'spastic dysphonia' will be used. the onset of this disorder is reported to be relatively abrupt and to die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 lesley wolk occur almost exclusively in middle age, with women being affected slightly more frequently than m e n . 1 6 ' 2 5 berendes has defined 'spastic dysphonia' as a disorder of phonation which is characterized by a strained, creaking and choked vocal attack, a tense and squeezed voice sound . . . accompanied by extreme tension of the entire phonatory system (cited in f o x , 1 2 p.275). he further noted that the disorder disturbs primarily the communicative function of speech, with other expressive functions of the voice (such as laughing, speaking to oneself or to animals) remaining intact. spastic dysphonia represents the most extreme form of overadduction of the vocal folds. there is effortful voice production, intermittent voice stoppage, a reduced vocal range and inappropriate change in pitch, quality and/or volume. 1 0 the patient may complain of 'a choking sensation' or a 'tightness of throat'. 7 several visible features as described, such as flushing of the face and frowning accompanying the vocal spasm and tic-like contractions of the neck muscles.1 , 2 ' 8 severe contractions of the thoracic and abdominal musculature accompany the laryngeal spasm and is probably a compensatory forcing response for overcoming the resistance to exhalation created by the glottal closure. these frustrated vocal efforts result in a pronounced alteration of pneumophonic (phonatory-breath) control. 1 1 ' 1 6 a basic controversy seems to emerge from the literature, reflecting two divergent schools of thought about the etiology of the disorder. one point of view, typified by brodnitz,6 suggests that spastic dysphonia is a psychogenic voice disorder, a form of conversion hysteria. here spastic dysphonia is seen as one of two extreme forms on the continuum of functional voice disorders: functional aphonia being the one extreme of hypofunction, and spastic dysphonia representing the extreme of hyperfunction. both disorders are interpreted as the physical manifestations of a deep-rooted emotional conflict.4' 1 3 luchsinger and a r n o l d 1 6 propose that this voice disorder may be explained as the intrusion of a subcortical primitive mechanism of phonation after the regular cortical phonatory system has become inhibited by the unconscious withdrawal of a shattered personality from the threats of daily life. in opposition to this point of view are others exemplified by robe, brumlik and m o o r e , 2 5 who claim that spastic dysphonia is a manifestation of disordered function of the central nervous system. this organic view was later advocated by aronson et al 2 who feel that spastic dysphonia has a neurologic substrate and may be related to the essential tremor syndrome. in view of the diversity of opinions regarding its etiology and intervention strategies, further research in this area seemed to be indicated. furthermore, the literature on this subject has been derived mainly from subjective-perceptual evaluations and qualitative descriptions. little attempt has been made to delineate the acoustic characteristics of spastic dysphonia or to describe the nature of the disorder by other objective means. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spastic dysphonia 5 in this paper, the author's intention is thus (a) to highlight the clinical picture of a case with spastic dysphonia and repaired cleft of the soft palate, (b) to attempt to delineate some of the acoustic parameters in this disorder of spastic dysphonia, and (c) to present some practical implications for diagnosis and treatment. case b a c k g r o u n d vocal history: mrs. g, aged 55 years, was first seen at the speech and hearing clinic, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg in june, 1979. she was referred by an ear, nose and throat (ent) specialist who reported a dysphonia with no organic pathology evident upon laryngeal examination. she reported that she suffered from a period of chronic laryngitis and experienced a sudden onset of voice difficulty approximately twelve months prior to the initial assessment. she stated that ί find difficulty getting out my words, especially if i'm a bit agitated. . . . i try to force my words out and i get so cross with myself'. she complained of no pain apart from some discomfort in the laryngeal region, fatigue of the upper chest muscles and severe tension in the abdominal area as she forces the air for speaking. she reported that conversational speech, telephone calls, reading long passages and supervising others at work were difficult situations for her, and that her voice deteriorated when she was agitated, angry or upset. there was evidence of moderate depression and extreme anxiety at the time of initial assessment. no previous speech assessments or treatment were reported. medical history: she was born with a cleft of the soft palate which was not repaired until recently (lips and hard palate were not affected). mrs. g. reported that although she has always had 'a nasal problem' related to the cleft, it never worried her. recently she decided to undergo surgery as she hoped that this would correct her voice problem. the velar cleft was successfully repaired in april, 1979. according to mrs. g, some improvement in her voice was noted in the immediate post-operative period, but a gradual deterioration has since occurred. she has undergone no other surgery, apart from a gynaecological operation in 1962. psychological and family history: mrs. g. described several significant 'family stress' factors which may be associated with the onset of her voice problem. no family history of speech, language or hearing problems was reported. she has been working for a major oil company for twelve years. at present she holds a senior position as a sub-accountant and appears to achieve job satisfaction. she has tended to avoid social contact since the onset of her voice difficulty. she described herself as a tense person and a perfectionist both at work and at home. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 lesley wolk clinical observations mrs. g. presents as an attractive, well-groomed, intelligent middle-aged woman. from an informal assessment, she displayed a reserved, controlled introvert-type personality. she exhibited traits of conscientiousness, keen awareness and discharge of her responsibilities. she maintains an upright, rigid posture with general body tension. extreme tension of her facial muscles, forehead and lips was noted to occur during speech; reflected in much frowning and facial contortions (while this is typical to spastic dysphonia,1 it may also be associated with inadequate velopharyngeal closure). these informal observations are consistent with damste 9 who characterizes patients with spastic dysphonia as reliable persons with strong sense of duty and a rigid personality, sensitive and easily hurt (p. 173). examinations c a r r i e d o u t examinations were carried out by an ent specialist, a neurologist and a psychologist respectively, in order to assist in making a differential diagnosis and to further provide evidence for the etiology of this disorder. results are summarized in table 1. table i : summary of findings from otological, neurological and psychological examinations. e x a m i n a t i o n f i n d i n g otological — no evidence of anatomical or functional disorder of larynx — normal vocal cord movement (indirect laryngoscopy) — no abnormality of tongue — slight nasal escape — difficulty in phonating neurological — no major neurological deficits apart from: — increased reflexes in upper and lower limbs — slightly awkward digital movements. — normal eeg — normal emi scan psychological — evidence of marital tension — strong sense of duty; rigid personality y — various sources of , emotional conflict the neurological findings seen in table i, bear some relationship to the findings of critchley8 and aronson et al1 which suggest evidence of hypertonus and hyperreflexia in patients with spastic dysphonia. no conclusions can be drawn in relation to the e e g findings as inconsistent results are reported in the literature. robe et a l 2 3 found that 90% of their subjects showed abnormal e e g recordings; whereas the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spastic dysphonia 7 aronson et al 1 found that 17 out of 22 patients showed normal e e g recordings, mild dysrhythmic activity in four others and independent spike foci in one patient. the findings from otological, neurological and psychological investigations seem to provide some support for the theory proposed by aronson and his co-workers.1 , 2 it is their belief that perhaps the type of personality described, superimposed on the 'unstable' motor system may be the adequate combination for the development of the disorder. they further suggest that these patients may have a basic predisposition or instability of innervation within the motor system, which is manifested through dysphonia when life events or personality abnormalities precipitate the symptoms. currently, d a m s t e 1 0 has found no evidence to assume that one single organic factor can be held responsible for causing spastic dysphonia. he contends that organic factors contribute in bringing the patients to a disadvantaged position. this, together with other external, constitutional and personality factors may trigger the 'pathologic defense system' of which he feels the symptoms of spastic dysphonia are a part. assessment o f communicaion b e h a v i o u r the assessment of communication behaviour was carried out by the author over a period of 3 months. this included a hearing assessment, an oral peripheral examination, a speech and language assessment. results are summarized in table ii. overall intelligibility thus appeared to be impaired by jerky rhythmic patterns, disturbed intonation patterns, periodic breaks in phonation and the frequent substitution or insertion of glottal stops. for the purpose of this paper, only a more detailed discussion of the acoustic impedance measurement will be included. acoustic impedance measurement studies have shown that middle ear muscle contraction precedes vocalisation by 65-100 ms or coincides with i t . 2 0 ' 2 6 the findings of recent studies provide evidence for a high incidence of apparent middle ear dysfunction in patients with spastic dysphonia.1 9 the use of acoustic impedance measurement has thus been advocated in order to further assess this phenomenon. briefly, the procedure used involved stimulating the acoustic reflex with sound stimuli at a 20 db suprareflex threshold level for approximately 30 seconds. the madsen z070 electroacoustic impedance bridge was used to observe the changes in middle ear impedance secondary to reflex contraction of the tympanic muscles. the focus in data analysis was concerned with certain dynamic response characteristics of the middle ear muscles as evidenced by the acoustic impedance measurements. particular attention was given to (1) on-time, (2) steady-state and (3) off-time characteristics of the acoustic reflex die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 lesley wolk table ii: summary of findings from an assessment of communication behaviour. hearing (a) pure-tone audiometry — normal (b) acoustic impedance audiometry — apparent abnormalities in middle ear muscle function (see below) oral peripheral examination (ope) — structural and functional adequacy of lips, teeth and jaw, tongue and hard palate. — slight asymmetry of soft palate and uvula. — soft palate is short — oropharynx is slightly shallow with excess width — nare constriction — adequate velopharyngeal closure since cleft repair — slight nasal escape (on plosives). speech — minimal articulation errors related to history of congenital velar cleft: — substitution and insertion of glottal stops. — distortion of fricatives (e.g. /s,z/). — substitution of voiceless sounds for their voiced cognates. language — normal response. a qualified audiologist acted as a second observer to provide a measure of reliability. the findings revealed normal tympanograms peaking at zero pressure level (mm w/s) bilaterally, and acoustic reflexes within normal limits. three types of apparent abnormalities in the middle ear muscle function were observed: 1. an apparent abnormality in the steady-state portion of the acoustic reflex was observed at 4 000 hz in both ears i.e. reflex contraction of the middle ear muscles was not sustained for the duration of acoustic stimulation at 4 000 hz. instead, the muscles apparently contracted briefly at the onset of stimulation and again at the cessation of stimulation. however, in view of the fact that this phenomenon only occurred at 4 000 hz, which has been found to occur in some normal subjects, 1 4 no conclusions can be drawn. 2. an abnormality in the off-time characteristics of the acoustic impedance response was noted to occur at 1 000 hz in the right (r) ear and at 500 hz in the left (l) ear. this refers to a prolonged time involved in the return to the baseline, which mccall 1 9 feels might be suggestive of a problem in muscle relaxation. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spastic dysphonia 9 3 there was evidence of possible tremor or shivering of the middle ear muscles at the following frequencies: 250 hz, 1 000 hz and 2 000 hz in the (r) ear, and 250 hz, 500 hz and 1 000 hz in the (l) ear. it is important to mention at this point, that these results may be affected by the presence of the velar cleft, as a high incidence of middle ear pathologies in cleft palate patients is well documented in the literature.3 nevertheless, the results discussed above are consistent with those of mccall 1 9 who found similar results in his patients with spastic dysphonia. the observation of possible difficulty in muscle relaxation and tremor of the middle ear muscles might add further support to suggest neurogenic involvement of the extrapyramidal motor system in this p a t i e n t . 1 9 damste 9 has further suggested that the combined activity of vocal cords, velum and middle ear muscles in normal speech, might serve to explain that all three may be affected simultaneously when there is a supernuclear excitation inhibition disorder. assessment o f v o i c e d i s o r d e r : subjective-perceptual evaluations an analysis of the tape recorded speech sample and general observations from the initial assessment revealed the following: 1. a disturbance of respiratory control and a lack of pneumophonic co-ordination. this is reflected by a predominant use of thoracic and clavicular breathing, shallow inhalations, jerky movements and a difficulty in sustaining phonation. 2. a variable pitch in conversational speech and a difficulty in regulating the pitch level. 3. a reduced intensity level. 4. a harsh voice quality with periodic breaks in phonation, accompanied by visible tension of the muscles in the face and neck. a glottal stroke was heard at the onset of phonation. there was evidence of apparent breathiness throughout all phonation. 5. the reflex actions of phonation were intact. thus, coughing, laughing and crying were carried out normally. singing, talking to herself and her pet cat were reported by her to facilitate improved phonation. this was confirmed by the clinician who listened to tape recordings of her speaking in these situations at home. objective evaluation — spectrographs assessment spectrographic analyses were carried out as a diagnostic tool in an attempt to gain further insight into the nature of this disorder. a kay sonagraph model 6061-b (kay elemetrics 6, pine brook, n.j.) was used to produce type b/65 spectrograms from the tape recordings of conversational speech. the data in this study were derived from broad-band and narrow-band spectrograms. practically, it was not possible to analyze spectrographically all utterances recorded during the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 lesley wolk diagnostic evaluations. thus only a few utterances from the spontaneous speech sample taken at the initial assessment, were randomly chosen. the analysis of spectrographic data was descriptive in nature. for the purpose of this paper, only a few of these utterances were selected in order to highlight the major findings. the spectrographic representation for the utterance 'and when i asked him' is illustrated in figure 1. on the broad-band spectrogram (fig. la) there is evidence of much breathiness and ill-defined harmonics. sections were taken during the steady-state of the vowel, which showed that the filter function is operating in such a way as to dampen the harmonics. spectral energy is entirely missing where formant 1 (fl) was expected. while f2 is present, there is no evidence of f1 and f3. this finding may be explained by a reduced driving force from the lungs, resulting in a reduced subglottal pressure at the vocal cords; 1 5 or possibly inadequate adjustment of the vocal folds which would also affect the range of harmonics. it can be noted in figure la that there are bursts of energy which probably occur after the initial glottal attack or 'laryngeal spasm'. the narrow-band spectrogram (fig. lb) confirms the breathiness evident on the broad-band spectrogram, reflecting a lack of vocal cord approximation. this is illustrated by a sporadic and inconsistent occurrence of harmonics. figure la. broad-band spectrogram showing utterance /and wen ai a:skt him/. n o t e : some of the noise evident is from the tape-recording. and wen ai ' a:skt him figure lb. narrow-band spectrogram showing utterance /and wen ai a:skt him/. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spastic dysphonia 1 1 the scale magnified narrow-band spectrograph^ representation of the ,,iterance ί asked my husband' is illustrated in figure 2. marked fluctuations in pitch can be seen on the vowel /a:/ in 'asked', with a relatively high fundamental frequency (±200 hz). there is a rise in oitch from 250 hz to 350 hz (100 hz in approximately 24 ms) indicating that pitch change is very rapid. it seems then, that rapid pitch fluctuations in this patient give rise, at least in part, to the perceptual effect of harshness.2 8 i ψ. t figure 2. scale-magnified narrow-band spectrogram showing utterance /ai a:skt mai tvizbond/. figure 3 represents the utterance 'how did the voice sound?' an examination of the scale magnified narrow-band spectrogram (fig. 3) alerted the author to another diagnostic feature which was hardly available during periodic subjective-perceptual evaluations. the harmonic structures represent the characteristic picture of diplophonia, which is clearly illustrated on the vowel /aw/ in 'sound'. here it can be seen that the harmonic at 1250 hz bears no relationship to the harmonic starting at 1000 hz and moving down to 800 hz. similar pitch fluctuations to those discussed in figure 2 are evident. it is interesting to note that there are simultaneous rising, falling and level pitches, indicating that the vocal cords are vibrating in at least two distinct modes at the same t i m e . 2 7 at the level pitch, the rate of vibration is maintained, whereas at the falling pitch there is a slowing down of the vocal fold vibration. it can be concluded therefore that in this case of diplophonia, there are (a) different modes of vibration and (b) different rates of vibration for each mode. b o o n e 5 has stated that diplophonia is frequently a symptom of general hyperfunctional usage of the vocal mechanism (p. 173). this diagnostic evaluation would thus seem to correlate significantly with the general diagnostic picture of spastic dysphonia and the frequent use of glottal stops resulting from the cleft palate. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 lesley wolk aw 31 /aw/ r figure 3. scale-magnified narrow-band spectrogram showing utterance /haw did after obtaining valuable diagnostic information from the spectrographic data discussed above, it was decided to extend the objective evaluation as a measure of therapeutic success. spectrographic analyses were carried out two months after the onset of treatment and at the end of treatment. the patient was treated for a total period of four months. utterances were again chosen randomly from taperecorded spontaneous speech samples. for the purpose of this discussion, spectrographic analyses carried out during the course of treatment will not be discussed in detail. they reflected a steady progression towards improved phonatory ability. figure 4 represents the utterance 'there were three factors', which was taken from a sample at the end of treatment. here it can be seen that there is some evidence of breathiness, although this is considerably reduced when compared with the utterance 'and when i asked him' in figure 1. the harmonics are clearly defined and there is no evidence of diplophonia. similar results were obtained from several other utterances analyzed spectrographically after treatment. the fact that the characteristic of 'diplophonia' was not evident on subjective-perceptual evaluations, leads one to assume that it may in fact be a symptom commonly associated with spastic dysphonia, but one which might have previously been overlooked because of the reliance upon subjective evaluations. da vsis sawnd/. v •mnaak / δ ε a w3: eri: fsekt3z figure 4. post-therapy scale-magnified narrow-band spectrogram showing utterance /δε3 w3: eri: fiektsz/1. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spastic dysphonia 13 p i h p r s c o p i c j ^ a m i n a t i o n : r t t j i z ^ ^ t r u m e n t a t i o n such as the flexible fiberoptic nasolaryngoscope (fiberscope) provides a means for making direct observations of laryngeal behaviour during voice production. the entire procedure was carried out in the department of phonetics and general linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, tohannesburg 2 7 the fiberscope was connected by a c-mount adapter to a camera. a diagnostic fiberoptic light source provided necessary illumination the fiberscope was inserted into one nostril until the tip of the scope reached a level slightly rostral to the superior margin of the epiglottis, which allowed for observation of the patient's vocal folds and laryngeal structures. a tape recorder uher 4200 report stereo ic with an m816 directional microphone, was prepared to run concurrently with this examination so as to record all utterances which could then be carefully examined with its corresponding slide. the patient was asked to (1) breathe at rest, (2) sustain voicing on the vowel /i:/, (3) converse with the investigator and (4) read a passage from a book. a review of the slides revealed the following findings: 1 the vocal cords were normal at rest, as illustrated in figure 5. at the onset of phonation for the vowel li:l there seemed to be a tight sphincteric closure, which subsequently disappeared and continued as normal voicing. during connected speech, there was evidence of an inconsistent picture, variable in the attitude of one vocal fold to the other, i.e. the two vocal folds did not lie equal to each other; one fold appeared larger and thicker as if it was pulled slightly over the other vocal fold. this is clearly illustrated in figure 6. 2. 3. figure 5. subject's laryngeal figure 6. subject's laryngeal bebehaviour during quiet breathhaviour during connected speech ing, showing that the vocal cords (voicing), are normal at rest. (note that the two vocal cords are not lying symmetrically to each other. the (r) fold is larger and thicker, as if it is pulled slightly over (l) fold. this picture of asymmetry was seen repeatedly in connected speech.) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 lesley wolk there was a general picture of 'shuddering' or 'laryngeal spasms' as expected for this voice disorder. throughout all conversational speech and reading tasks, a slight 'chink' in the vocal cords was evident, which would account for the continual breathy escape. overall results thus served to confirm the diagnosis of 'diplophonia' noted on the spectrographic assessment; and to validate the information obtained from subjective-perceptual evaluations. in summary, subjective-perceptual evaluations of the voice revealed a strained, harsh-breathy voice quality with frequent breaks in phonation, a variable pitch and visible tension in the face and neck muscles. objective spectrographic evaluations revealed much turbulence, illdefined harmonics, a breakdown in formant structure ('filtering-out' of formants), rapid pitch fluctuations and evidence of diplophonia which was confirmed on the fiberscopic examination. post-treatment spectrographic evaluations indicated an improved phonatory ability. there was a minimum turbulence, clearly defined harmonics, increased energy, increased sub-glottal pressure and an absence of diplophonia; giving rise to a less breathy, less harsh, less choked voice quality. this was combined with improved pneumophonic co-ordination, reduced tension of the face and neck muscles, an elimination of glottal attacks and a better adjusted social personality. conclusions a n d implications it seems premature to make a general assumption that a patient with spastic dysphonia is suffering either from a psychogenic disorder or a neurologic one. as aronson et al 1 proposed, the issue must be resolved on the basis of findings that can be established for each particular patient. the author has thus postulated that in this single case, the onset of spastic dysphonia may be attributed to long term vocal misuse in compensation for the velar c l e f t , 1 8 ' 2 1 possibly precipitated by the physical trauma of 'secondary laryngitis'1 6 and the cleft repair operation, combined with the psychological stresses of life which occur during middle age. furthermore both the neurological and psychological findings in this study might point to an underlying organic or psychogenic predisposition. this explanation is supported by cooper 7 who recently concluded that: the onset and development of incipient spastic dysphonia and spastic dysphonia are due to long term/vocal misuse, with psychological or physical trauma often being the'catalyst (p. 173). an extension of this may lead one to postulate that in this single case, both the congenital velar cleft and the predisposition for the occurrence of spastic dysphonia may be related to a single common constitutional or embryological factor. ι clinical implications 1 the spectrographic analyses and fiberscopic examination carried out in this study served to provide an accurate diagnosis of the voice disorder the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spastic dysphonia 15 and an evaluation of voice improvement both during and after treatment. most authors agree that the overall prognosis in patients with spastic dysphonia is p o o r . 5 , 7 currently however, it is still felt that the combination of voice therapy with psychotherapy offers the best chances for i m p r o v e m e n t . 4 , 6 several voice therapy techniques have been advocated, some of which were found to be useful in the treatment of this patient. these include procedures such as physical relaxation, respiration training and the correction of pneumophonic c o o r d i n a t i o n ; 7 , 1 0 , 1 3 the yawn-sigh technique, humming and vowel exercises, froeschel's chewing method, the elimination of hard glottal attacks and the use of a hierarchy analysis to assist the patient in recreating those environments where relaxed phonation is best a c h i e v e d . 5 , 2 5 e m g biofeedback has recently been proposed as an effective adjunct to traditional therapy methods for hyperfunctional voice disorders.2 2 its applicability and effectiveness in patients with spastic dysphonia, however, is still uncertain, so that further investigation into this area seems to be indicated. the findings of this study point to an important implication regarding habilitative procedures employed in the treatment of cleft palate children. there seems to be a tendency in some cases to overlook the vocal dimension in the treatment of these patients, focusing mainly on the improvement of velopharyngeal closure and increased flexibility of the articulators. luse, heisse and f o l e y 1 7 showed that all their cleft palate cases had a spasm of the hypopharynx prior to rehabilitation, indicating excessive hypopharyngeal and laryngeal tension. they thus contend that cleft palate quality can be eliminated by reducing-tension in the laryngeal and pharyngeal areas, so that the manner of phonation or vocalisation would seem to be the starting point for rehabilitation. while this study was concerned primarily with an evaluation of the vocal parameters, it would be of interest and probably essential to further investigate speech breathing in patients with spastic dysphonia. further research employing spectrographic analyses and fiberscopic examinations, working optimally within a team approach, would be of value in the evaluation and rehabilitation of a variety of voice disorders. acknowledgements the author wishes to thank mrs. m. wahlhaus, senior lecturer and ms. c. penn, lecturer, in the department of speech pathology and audiology of the university of the witwatersrand for their invaluable guidance and support, and mr. a. traill, senior lecturer of the department of phonetics and general linguistics for his valuable assistance in the spectrographic and fiberscopic testing of this patient. the author also acknowledges the human sciences research council for its financial assistance. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommnikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 lesley wolk references 1. aronson, a. e . , brown, j. r., litin, ε. m. and pearson, j. s. (1968): spastic dysphonia i. voice, neurologic and psychiatric aspects. j. speech hear. dis. 33, 203-218. 2. aronson, a. e., brown, j. r., litin, ε. m. and pearson, j. s. (1968): spastic dysphonia ii. comparison with essential (voice) tremor and other neurologic and psychogenic dysphonias. j. speech hear dis. 33, 219-231. 3. arora, μ. m. l., sharma, v. l., gudi, s. p. and balakrishnan, c. (1979): acoustic impedance measurements and their importance in cleft palate patients. the j. laryngol and otol. 93, 443-445. 4. bloch, p. (1965): neuro-psychiatric aspects of spastic dysphonia. folio phoniat. 17, 301-364. 5. boone, d. r. (1971): the voice and voice therapy. prentice-hall, inc. englewood cliffs, new jersey. 6. brodnitz, f. s. (1976): spastic dysphonia. ann. otol. 85, 210-214. 7. cooper, m. (1973): modern techniques of vocal rehabilitation. charles, c. thomas publishers, springfield, illinois. 8. critchley, m. (1939): spastic dysphonia; ('inspiratory speech'). brain. 62, 96-103. 9. damste, p. h. (1977): spastic dysphonia; untieing or cutting the knot. o.r.l. 39(3), 173. 10. damste, p. h. (1978): spastic dysphonia: an interpretation of the symptoms. communicative disorders in audio journal for continuing education. 3(5) may. bradford, l. j. (ed) grune and stratton, inc., new york. 11. dedo, η. h. (1976): recurrent laryngeal nerve section for spastic dysphonia. ann. otol. 85, 451-459. 12. fox, d. r. (1969): spastic dysphonia: a case presentation. j. speech hear. dis. 34, 275-279. 13. greene, m. c. l. (1964): the voice and its disorders. pitman medical publishers, co., ltd., 2nd edition. 14. jerger, j. (ed). (1973): modern developments in audiology. 2nd edition academic press inc. a subsidiary of harcourt, brace jovanovich. publishers. 15. ladefoged, p. (1967): three areas of experimental phonetics. chapter 1. oxford university press. ely house, l o n d o n ' 16. luchsinger, r., and arnold, g. e. (1965): voice-speech-language. belmont, california. wordsworth. 17. luse, e., heisse, j. and foley, j. (1964): the vocal approach in the correction of cleft palate speech.[folio phoniat. 16, 123-129. 18. marks, c. r., barker, k. and tardy; μ. e. (1971): prevalence of perceived acoustic deviations related' to laryngeal function among subjects with palatal abnormalities. cleft palate j. 8(2) 201-211. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spastic dysphonia 17 19. mccall, g. n. (1973):. acoustic impedance measurement in the study of patients with spasmodic dysphonia. j. speech hear. dis. 38(2), 250-255. 20. mccall, g. n. and rabuzzi, d. d . (1973): reflex contraction of middle ear muscles secondary to stimulation of laryngeal nerves. j. speech hear res. 16, 56-61. 21. mcdonald, ε. t. and koepp-baker, h. (1951): cleft palate speech. an interpretation of research and clinical observations. /. speech hear dis. 16, 9-20. 22. prosek, r. α., montgomery, α. α., walden, β. e. and schwartz, d. m. (1978): emg biofeedback in the treatment of hyperfunctional voice disorders. j. speech. hear. dis. 43(3), 282-294. 23. robe, e . , brumlik, j. and moore, p. (1960): a study of spastic dysphonia: neurologic and electroencephalographic abnormalities. laryngoscope. 70(3), 219-245. 24. saito, s., fukuda, h., kitahara, s. and kokawa, n. (1978): stroboscopic observation of vocal fold vibration with fiberoptics. folio phoniat. 30(4) 241-244. 25. segre, r. (1951): spasmodic aphonia. folio phoniat. 3, 150-157. 26. shearer, w. m., and simmons, f. b. (1965): middle ear activity during speech in normal speakers and stutterers. j. speech hear. res. 8, 203-207. 27. traill, a. (1979): senior lecturer in the department of phonetics and general linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. personal communication. 28. wendahl, r. w. (1963): laryngeal analog synthesis of harsh voice quality. folio phoniat. 15, 241-250. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) we invite you to talk to the firm who supplied the equipment you were originally trained on for all your hearing and acoustic requirements. audiometers train ears hearing test rooms hearing aids phonic ear f.m. radio systems speech therapy equipment needler westdene organisation (pty) ltd p.o. box 28975, , phone: 011 45?7262 sandringham, 01145-6113/4. johannesburg, 2131. i the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 45 ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie: aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking a van der merwe, d phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria i c uys, d phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria j μ loots, d sc (fisiologie) (pretoria) instituut vir sportnavorsing, universiteit van pretoria r j grimbeek, β sc (hons) (pretoria) departement statistiek, universiteit van pretoria opsomming baie vrae bestaan nog oor die aard en oorsake van diesimptome van verbale apraksie. tydens 'n ondersoek na dieinvloed van kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembafe~foute by verbale apraksie (van der merwe, uys, loots en grimbeek, 1987) is simptome wat aanduidings verskaf-van'die aard van die afwyking waargeneem. hierdie simptome word verder geanaliseer in hierdie artikel. die frekwensiferttipe foute by herhaalde produksie van onsineenhede wat sistematies gevarieer is in klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe is nagegaan by vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en een persoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie. daar is onder andere waargeneem dat suiwer verbale apraksie gekenmerk word deur distorsie van klanke wat soms soos vervangings klink, afbakening van lettergrepe en veranderinge in die struktuur van 'n eenheid met veral 'n neiging om dit te vereenvoudig na 'n kv-eenheid of duplisering daarvan. daar is moontlik 'n onderliggende onvermoe om die kritiese ekwivalensiegrense en bewegings te handhaaf, klanke vinnig en onveranderlik vooriiit te voer en om spraak vooruit te beplan. abstract / many questions on the nature and causes of the symptoms of verbal apraxia still exist. in the investigation of the influence of contextual factors on auditorily perceived symptoms of verbal apraxia (van der merwe, uys, loots and grimbeek, 1987) symptoms indicative of the nature of the disorder were recorded and these are further analysed in this article. the frequency and nature of mistakes made during repeated productions of nonsense units which are systematically varied in sound structure and articulatory features were determined in the speech of four patientsj with acquired verbal apraxia and one patient with developmental verbal apraxia. the most prominent indications were that pure verbal apraxia is characterised by sound distortions whica are..sometimes perceived as substitutions, intersyllabic pauses and changes in the sound structure of units manifesting a tendency/o amplify and change the structure to a cv-unit or reduplication thereof. there possibly is an underlying inability to keep movements within the critical boundaries of motor equivalence to quickly and invariantly feed sounds forward and to preplan speech. navorsing oor verbale apraksie is in die verlede hoofsaaklik gerig op simptoombeskrywings en die kenmerkende simptome van suiwer verbale apraksie is mettertyd gei'dentifiseer. daar was aanvanklik geen eenstemmigheid oor die simptome van die afwyking nie en die suiwer vorm is selfs nog gedurende die vroee tagtigerjare deur sommige navorsers nie onderskei van broca afasie en fonemiese parafasie nie (martin, 1974; keller, 1978; miceli, gainotti, caltagirone & masullo, 1980; guyard, sabouraud & gagnepain, 1981). die implikasie van hierdie verwarring was dat simptome van afasie aan verbale apraksie toegeskryf is en sodoende is kennis en navorsing oor hierdie omstrede en komplekse afwyking benadeel. die oortuiging dat daar wel 'n suiwer vorm van verbale apraksie is, het mettertyd meer algemeen posgevat en in 1983 stel kent en rosenbek en ook collins, rosenbek en wertz kriteria vir die seleksie van hul verbaal apraktiese proefpersone ten einde te verseker dat 'n suiwer vorm van verbale apraksie ondersoek word. hierdie kriteria kom opsommend daarop neer dat die sprekers onvlot spraak, pr6beeren-tref-artikulasiebewegings, pogings tot selfkorreksie, onkonstantheid in die foute by herhaalde produksie van dieselfde woord en disprosodie vertoon, maar fraselengte en grammatikale vorm is normaal of byna normaal. deur te verseker dat die afwyking in sy suiwer vorm ondersoek word, word die weg gebaan om die aard van verbale aprakdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 © sasha 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46· a van der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots en r j grimbeek sie werklik bloot te le deur navorsing. talle vrae oor die aard van verbale apraksie bestaan tans nog. verbale apraksie word algemeen in definisies as 'n afwyking in spraakprogrammering beskryf, maar in werklikheid is daar baie min bekend oor wat spraakprogrammering werklik behels. darley, aronson & brown (1975 : 255) definieer verbale apraksie as: "... an articulatory disorder resulting from impairment, due to brain damage, of the capacity to program the positioning of speech muscalature for the volitional production of phonemes and the sequencing of muscle movements for the production of words". programmering word in hierdie definisie dus beperk tot posisionering van strukture en die volgordebepaling van bewegings. dit is ook nie duidelik in die definisie of die programmeringsprobleem 'n afwyking in die beplanning van die aksie is of ' η afwyking wat intree tydens die uitvoer van die beweging nie. la pointe (1982 : 7) definieer verbale apraksie as: "... a neurogenic phonologic disorder resulting from sensori-motor impairment of the capacity to select, program and/or execute in coordinated and normally-timed sequences, the positioning of the speech musculature for the volitional production of speech sounds". in hierdie definisie word onderskei tussen seleksie, programmering en uitvoering. die implikasie hiervan is dat programmering nie seleksie insluit nie. dit is ook nie volkome duidelik wat gekoordineer moet word nie. buckingham het reeds in 1979 die vaagheid van terme soos programmering, wankoordinering en volgorde gekritiseer. die onsekerheid oor wat spraakprogrammering of beplanning behels en wat die aard van 'n afwyking daarin is, hang nou saam met die vraag oor die stadium van spraakproduksie wat defek is by verbale apraksie. tydens spraakproduksie is daar waarskynlik sprake van fonologiese en in 'n volgende stadium van motoriese beplanning van 'n uiting. die fonologiese eienskappe van 'n uiting is 'n linguistiese komponent van spraakproduksie. navorsers met 'n linguistiese orientasie is van mening dat spraak en taal interafhanklik en onafskeidbaar is (jakobson, fant & halle, 1967; martin, 1974) en die fonologiese komponent verteenwoordig die tussenvlak tussen spraakproduksie (fonetiese aspekte) en taal (edwards & shriberg, 1983). differensiele aantasting van of taal of spraak is dus volgens die unguis nie moontlik nie (martin, 1974). vanuit so 'n uitgangspunt is verbale apraksie as 'n fonologiese probleem beskou en in die literatuur het die dispuut oor die aard van die afwyking dan ook hoofsaaklik gesentreer om die vraagstuk van verbale apraksie as 'n "motoriese teenoor 'n fonologiese probleem (martin, 1974; aten, darley, deal & johns, 1975; kent & rosenbek, 1983). wesenlik handel dit dus oor die skeiding van linguistiese (fonologiese) en motoriese gedrag. in hul bespreking oor die kwessie van verbale apraksie as 'n motoriese teenoor 'n fonologiese probleem, se kent en rosenbek (1983: 245) dat minstens sommige van die foute: "... do not seem to fall in the domain of phonology because as the term is commonly understood, phonology is not intended to explain the details of timing and coordination in speech". hulle se verder ook dat hulle nie daarop aanspraak maak dat verbale apraksie slegs 'n motoriese probleem is nie en dat dit montlik is dat beide tipes probleme saam voorkom. in die bogenoemde definisie van la pointe (1982) omseil hy die twispunt deur dit 'n fonologiese afwyking te noem, maar dan 'n beskrywing te gee van die motoriese probleme. itoh en sy medewerkers (itoh, sasanuma & ushijima, 1979; itoh & sasanuma, 1984) wat intensiewe ondersoeke oor die temporale versteuring van spraakbewegings by verbale apraksie gedoen het, stel dit onomwonde dat verbale apraksie na hul mening 'n afwyking in programmering van spraakmotoriek is. volgens hierdie navorsers is distorsie van klanke die kernsimptoom van verbale apraksie en is dit die gevolg van temporale versteurings van spraakbewegings. vroeer is algemeen aanvaar dat klankvervangings die kenmerkendste simptoom van verbale apraksie is (shankweiler & harris, 1966; la pointe & johns, 1975; keller, 1978; wertz, la pointe & rosenbek, 1984: 52). dit is waarskynlik omdat die gevalle wat ondersoek is, ook gepaardgaande broca afasie en/of fonemiese parafasie en dus ware klankvervangings vertoon het en omdat bree fonetiese transkripsies gebruik is om die spraakfoute te beskryf (itoh & sasanuma, 1984). op grond van hierdie transkripsiemetode word distorsies dus as vervangings beskryf. deur gebruik te maak van eng fonetiese transkripsies, soos in hierdie studie beskryf word, kan distorsiefoute beter nagegaan word. tydens die ondersoek na die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie (van der merwe, uys, loots & grimbeek, 1987) is simptome waargeneem wat aanduidings verskaf van die aard van die afwyking maar wat nie in daardie artikel volledig aangegee is nie. die doel van die huidige artikel is om hierdie addisionele waarnemings te beskryf en te wys op die implikasies wat dit inhou vir 'n beter begrip van die aard van verbale apraksie. heelwat ander persept'uele studies oor die simptome van verbale apraksie is in: die verlede uitgevoer (la pointe & johns, 1975; johns & darley, 1970, dunlop & marquardt, 1977). hierdie studie is egter uniek in die sin dat die materiaal volkome gekontroleer'd is in klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe, 'n eng transkripsiemetode gebruik is wat ook voorsiening maak vir'die voorkoms van klankdistorsie en vier agtereenvolgende herhalings van die onsineenhede ontleed is. hierdie faktore bring mee dat die waarnemings binne hoogs gekontroleerde ' toestande kan plaasvind en ruimte gelaat word vir volledige beskrywings van gedrag. metode χ doel om te bepaal wat die frekwensie en tipe verbaal apraktiese foute is wat voorkom by herhaalde produksies van onsinhede wat sistematies gevarieer is in klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe ten einde aanduidings te verkry van die aard van die afwyking by verbale apraksie. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie: aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking 47 eksperimentele ontwerp die proefpersone, luisteraars, materiaal en prosedure van hierdie ondersoek is reeds volledig beskryf in die voorafgaande artikel (van der merwe et al. 1987) eri word dus nie hier herhaal nie. kortliks kan wel gemeld word dat vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en een persoon met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie as proefpersone gebruik is. die kriterium is gestel dat 'n suiwer verbale apraksie vertoon moet word. wat die materiaal betref, is onsineenhede wat gekontroleer is in klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe ontwikkel (van der merwe, 1986). die volgende vyf klankstruktuurgroepe (s), het elk agt eenhede bevat en elke eenheid is vier keer herhaal: struktuurgroep 1: κι vi k1 v2 struktuurgroep 2: κι vi k2 v2 struktuurgroep 3: κι vi k2 v2 k3 struktuurgroep 4: κι vi k2 struktuurgroep 5: κι vi k2 v2 k3 v3 k4 en κι vi k2 v2 k3 v2 k4 vir die analise van die data is 'n lys saamgestel van alle spraakfoute wat voorgekom het by die vyf proefpersone. hierdie foute is geklassifiseer in die volgende sewe foutkategoriee: a. vervangings b. verandering van die struktuur van die eenheid c. klankdistorsie d. afwykings in temporale vloei e. afwykings in vlotheid f. spraakfoute gevolg deur selfkorreksie g. afwykings in prosodie die tipe foute in elke foutkategorie verskyn volledig in die bylae van die vorige artikel en in tabelle 1 tot 7 van hierdie artikel. j 7 die somtotaal van elke vjerskillende tipe fout oor vier herhalings van 'n spesifieke eenheid is bepaal vir elke proefpersoon. die totale aantal kere wat 'n spesifieke fout in elke klankstruktuurgroep voorkom, is daarna bereken en hierdie totale word in tabelle ljtot 7 aangegee. 'n analise van die data het getoon dat artikulasie-eienskappe nie 'n statisties aantoonbare effek het op die tipe foute wat voorkom nie (van der merwe, 1986; van der merwe et al. 1987) en daarom word slegs die tipe foute by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe bespreek. resultate en bespreking afwykings in temporale vloei het die hoogste frekwensie van voorkoms, naamlik 35,6% vertoon en daarna distorsiefoute (30,7%), verandering van struktuur (9,6%), afwykings in prosodie (9,6%), vervangings (8,5%), afwykings in vlotheid (4,7%) en foute gevolg deur selfkorreksie (1,3%). elk van hierdie foutkategoriee word vervolgens bespreek. vervangings die opsommende gegewens in tabel 1 toon aan dat vervangings met eenheidskonsonante (konsonante wat in die doelwiteenheid teenwoordig is) die meeste voorkom. dit is veral by s3 en s5 wat oor die algemeen die meeste foute tot gevolg gehad het, waargeneem. by klankstruktuur 5 is daar 75 vervangings met eenheidskonsonante maar daar is geen sodanige foute by si en s4 nie. 'n vervanging met 'n eenheidskonsonant impliseer dat die volgorde van klanke in die eenheid foutief is. dit weerspieel dus klank volgordeningsprobleme en nie seleksieprobleme nie. klankstrukture 3 en 5 is die langste eenhede en stel dus die hoogste eise met betrekking tot die volgordening van klanke asook geheue vir die klanke wat voorgekom het in die eenheid. die spreker selekteer dus die korrekte foneem, maar kombineer dan die doelwitklanke in die verkeerde volgorde. hierdie simptoom kan as 'n fonologiese probleem geklassifiseer word omdat dit 'n afwyking in fonologiese beplanning weerspieel. dit is egter belangrik om daarop te wys dat die probleem nie konstant teenwoordig was nie. tydens herhaalde produksie van dieselfde uiting was die eenheid soms korrek en soms nie. dit is onwaarskynlik dat fonologiese kennis so labiel kan wees en die simptoom' weerspieel dus waarskynlik nie 'n afwyking in fonologiese kennis nie maar eerder 'n probleem wat ontstaan tydens produksie. 'n vraag wat ontstaan, is of die beeld van die uiting soos wat die ondersoeker dit geproduseer het nie vervaag in die geheue van die verbaal apraktiese spreker en dan lei tot foutiewe volgordening van konsonante en vervangings met eenheidskonsonante nie. dit is egter nie die geval nie, want dit was 'n algemene verskynsel dat die eerste herhalings foutief was, maar die laaste een dan weer korrek. die persoon behou dus 'n beeld van wat geproduseer moet word, maar by herhaalde produksie is dit onkonstant korrek. die feit dat die uiting soms korrek is dui sterk daarop dat die tabel 1: vervangingsfoute by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe: klankstrukvervangingsfoute tuurgroepe konsonant vokaal eenheidsnie-eeneenheidsnie-eentotaal klank heidsklank klank heidsklank 1 0 8 1 16 0 25 (11,7%) 2 3 3 1 15 0 22 (10,3%) 3 19 14 0 2 2 37 (17,3%) 4 0 4 0 0 0 4 ( 1,9%) 5 75 24 7 16 4 126 (58,8%) totaal 1 9 7 53 9 49 6 2 1 4 (45,3%) (24,8%) (4,2%) (22,9%) (2,8%) (100%)^ die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 a van der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots en r j grimbeek spreker 'n duidelike beeld het van wat verwag word en ook oor die nodige kennis beskik om die uiting fonologies te beplan, maar nie altyd in staat is om dit korrek vooruit te voer vir produksie nie. die vervangings met eenheidskonsonante het meestal meegebring dat 'n besondere klank herhaal word in die eenheid. guyard et al. (1981) het dieselfde simptoom waargeneem by gevalle met broca afasie en gepaardgaande verbale apraksie en hulle interpreteer dit as 'n vereenvoudiging van die reeks. die duplisering van klanke bring waarskynlik fonologiese en motoriese vereenvoudiging mee en dit is dus op grond van so 'n verduideliking nie moontlik om die simptoom as 'n motoriese of 'n fonologiese probleem te klassifiseer nie. dit is belangrik om daarop te wys dat dit veraljproefpersone 5 en 3 was wat vervangings vertoon het. in die geval van proefpersoon 5 kan die probleem moontlik verband hou met die feit dat hy 'n verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie het en dat hy moontlik 'n besondere probleem met volgordening van klanke het. die simptome wat hy openbaar, stem egter heeltemal ooreen met die simptome van proefpersoon 3. dieselfde tipe vervangings het by beide voorgekom. proefpersoon 4 het ook dikwels vervangings vertoon terwyl dit selde voorgekom het by proefpersone 1 en 2. klankvervangings is dus nie 'n kenmerkende simptoom van verbale apraksie_nie. indien die oorsprong daarvan wel suiwer fonologies (dit wil se linguisties-simbolies) is, impliseer die lae voorkoms daarvan (8,5% van alle foute) by slegs sommige verbaal apraktiese sprekers dat die fonologies gebaseerde simptome nie 'n integrerende deel van verbale apraksie is nie. vervangings met nie-eenheidsklanke, wat moontlik op foutiewe seleksie van klanke dui, het in mindere mate as vervangings met eenheidsklanke voorgekom. die hoogste frekwensie was ook by klankstrukture 3 en 5. hierdie vervangings kan ook as paradigmatiese foute beskryf word omdat dit foutiewe seleksie vanuit 'n interne "stoor" van foneme impliseer (jakobson, 1971; keller, 1978). vanuit hierdie gesigspunt is vervangings met nie-eenheidsklanke suiwer fonologies gebonde probleme. die besondere eise van die langer eenhede veroorsaak dat hierdie probleme intree. 'n nadere ondersoek van die spesifieke klanke wat vervang word, werp egter 'n ander lig op sommige van die vervangings met nie-eenheidskonsonante. die ibl is byvoorbeeld herhaaldelik vervang met iml. hierdie klanke is artikulatories verwant en die moontlikheid is nie uitgesluit dat die iml in werklikheid 'n distorsie in die produksie-eienskappe van ibl is nie. die luisteraars het moontlik die klank dan as 'n iml geperseptueer. vokaalvervangings is ook by hierdie groep sprekers waargeneem. dit bevestig die resultate van keller (1978) wat ook vokaalvervangings by persone met broca afasie en gepaardgaande verbale apraksie gevind het. slegs nege vokaalvervangings met eenheidsklanke het voorgekom en sewe daarvan was by s5. dit wil dus voorkom asof dit 'n ongewone verskynsel is by verbale apraksie en dat die persoon bewus is van die vokale in die uiting en die posisie daarvan. die volgordening van konsonante is dus 'n groter probleem. die vokaalvervangings met nie-eenheidsklanke was in die meeste gevalle vervangings van hi vir id en lo:l vir hi. oppervlakkig beskou, dui dit op fonologiese seleksieprobleme, maar daar is 'n paar aanduidings dat 'n groot persentasie van hierdie vervangings in werklikheid die gevolg van probleme in motoriese programmering is. daar is in die eerste plek waargeneem dat dieselfde vervangings by al die proefpersone voorgekom het. dit impliseer dat 'n gemeenskaplike probleem of eienskap tot die simptoom aanleiding gee. vanuit die oogpunt van 'n gemerktheidsanalise dui bogenoemde vervangings op vereenvoudiging na laer gemerkte vokale (keller, 1978). die bewegings word dus sistematies vereenvoudig. dit kan ook wees dat die artikulatoriese grense (ruimtelik en temporaal) tussen hierdie vokale nie streng afgebaken is deur die sprekers nie en dat geringe afwykings in die kritiese bewegingspesifikasies daartoe gelei het dat die luisteraars die afwykings as vervangings perseptueer terwyl dit in werklikheid 'n vorm van distorsie is. die onkonstante aard van die vervangings dui ook daarop dat dit die gevolg is van klankdistorsie en waarskynlik weens 'n oorskryding van die kritiese artikulasiegrense tussen klanke. 'n eenheid soos idokd is byvoorbeeld afwisselend geproduseer as /doke:, doki, dcfe/. dit is dus onwaarskynlik die gevolg van 'n afwyking in fonologiese klankseleksie. lettergreepvervangings het slegs voorgekom by s3 en s5. indien aanvaar word dat hierdie klankstrukture die moeilikste vir die verbaal apraktiese spreker is om te produseer, is lettergreepvervangings moontlik 'n ernstiger afwyking. dit het egter selde voorgekom by die verbaal apraktiese sprekers. opsommend kom die resultate daarop neer dat klankvervangings wel voorkom by verbaal apraktiese sprekers, maar nie in dieselfde mate by almal nie. die meeste vervangings kom voor by s5 wat die langste klankstruktuur het. dit wil dus voorkom asof vervangings konteks-sensitief is (van der merwe et al. 1987). vervangings is egter nie 'n prominente simptoom van verbale apraksie nie want slegs 8,5% van al die foute was klankvervangings. die vervangings wat wel voorkom is waarskynlik nie weens 'n fonologiese beplanningsprobleem nie, want dit kom onkonstant voor tydens herhaalde produksie. dit is ook nie weens 'n geheueof fonologiese herroepings-probleem nie want die laaste van die herhalings was soms fonologies die mees korrekte. dit wil wel voorkom asof daar 'n probleem is in die onveranderlike vooruitvoering van foneme in die korrekte volgorde. daar is veral 'n neiging om konsonante te vervang met eenheidskonsonante wat dus 'n afwyking in klankvolgorde is. moontlik word die fonologiese en motoriese komponente van die produksie van die eenheid sodoende vereenvoudig. vervangings met nie-eenheidskonsonante en vokale kom ook voor maar by 'n nadere ondersoek van die artikulasieeienskappe van die spesifieke klanke blyk dit dat hietdie vervangings meer waarskynlik distorsies van die doelwitklank is wat luisteraars perseptueer as vervangings. verandering van struktuur van eenhede / 'n totaal van 241 (9,6%) foute is in hierdie foutkategorie aangeteken. die hoogste persentasie (38,2%).foute het by s3 en s5 voorgekom (kyk tabel 2). dit is interessant dat dieselfde getal foute by altwee klankstrukture voorgekom het. die gevolgtrekking dat die lengte van 'n uiting nie die enigste faktor is wat die mate van afwyking bepaal nie (van der merwe et the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie: aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking tabel 2: verandering van struktuur by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe verandering van struktuur van eenhede klankstruktuurgroepe totaal verandering van struktuur van eenhede si s2 s3 s4 s5 totaal byvoeging van κ in mediale posisie 29(12,0%) (eenheidsklank) 1 13 15 29(12,0%) byvoeging van κ in mediale posisie 32(13,3%) (nie-eenheidsklank) 2 7 7 5 11 32(13,3%) byvoeging van κ in finale posisie 6 (2,5%) (eenheidsklank) 4 2 6 (2,5%) byvoeging van κ in finale posisie 5 (2%) (nie-eenheidsklank) 2 2 1 5 (2%) byvoeging van v (eenheidsklank) 0 (0%) byvoeging van v (nie-eenheidsklank) 2 2 32(13,3%) weglating van κ 4 23 5 32(13,3%) weglating van v 7 7 (2,9%) byvoeging van lettergreep 1 4 5 (2%) weglating van lettergreep 2 9 11 (4,6%) weglating van alle konsonante en 1 (0,4%) slegs vokaalproduksie 1 1 (0,4%) omruiling van klanke 1 1 (0,4%) struktuur verander na kv 2 2 (0,8%) struktuur verander na kvkv 17 2 19 (7,9%) struktuur verander na kvk 0 (0%) struktuur verander na kvkvk 1 1 2 4 (1,6%) struktuur verander na kvkvkv 1 1 3 5 (2,0%) struktuur verander na kvkvkvk 1 1 (0,4%) struktuur verander na ander struktuur 2 8 18 32 60(24,9%) afbreking binne geslote lettergreep 5 8 4 17 (7%) totaal 12 28 92 17 92 241 (5%) (11,6%) "(38,2%) (7%) (38,2%) (100%) nege keer voorgekom) of enkelklanke. die struktuur is soms verleng en dit het meegebring dat dit in werklikheid moeiliker gemaak is. geen strategiee om die uiting te vereenvoudig kan hierin gevind word nie en die indruk word gewek dat die spreker nie die eenheid kan onthou nie. in baie gevalle was die laaste herhalings egter nader aan die doelwituiting wat die moontlikheid aandui dat die kombinasie van foneme en/of die onveranderlike vooruitvoering daarvan problematies is by langer uitings. 'n verder interessante verskynsel is die afbreking binne geslote lettergrepe wat by s3, s4 en s5 voorgekom het. die redenasie kan moontlik geopper word dat dit eerder as afwykings in temporale vloei geklassifiseer moet word. dit was egter die afbakening van lettergrepe binne die uiting wat verander is. die kvk-eenhede is byvoorbeeld verander na kv-k en die kvkvk-eenhede na kvkv-k. hierdie verskynsel dui ook daarop dat die vooruitvoering van 'n kv-, of kvkv-eenheid wat 'n duplisering is, makliker of meer natuurlik is en dat die uiting sodoende vereenvoudig word. verandering van struktuur het by al die proefpersone voorgekom, maar daar was individuele gedragspatrone wat baie interessante inligting aan die lig gebring het. proefpersoon 2 het 'n feitlik konstante getal foute by al die strukture vertoon behalwe by s4 waar daar minder was. proefpersone 3 en 5 het dieselfde tipe foute vertoon en beide het veral by s3 en s5 foute gemaak. proefpersoon 1 het onverwags by s4 foute vertoon en proefpersoon 4 het geen foute gemaak nie. die huidige data wat verkry is, kan slegs in beperkte mate met die bevindinge van ander navorsers vergelyk word. la al. 1987) word dus deur hierdie analise bevestig. by s3 is die struktuur 17 keer vereenvoudig na 'n kvkv-struktuur en 18 veranderings na ander strukture soos veral 'n kvkkvkstruktuur het voorgekom. dit wil dus voorkom asof die struktuur van s3 moeilik is vir die verbaal apraktiese spreker. dit is waarskynlik so dat die, herbeplande struktuur makliker vooruitgevoer kan word. die verandering na 'n kvkv-struk/tuur is ondersteuning vir die teorie dat die kv-eenheid die natuurlike eenheid van spraakprogrammering is (jakobson, 1971: 21), maar die verandering na 'n kvkkvk-struktuur is in opposisie met die teorie1. laasgenoemde is wel 'n duplisering van die kvk-eenheid wat min afwykings tot gevolg gehad het. die feit dat die klankstruktuur herbeplan en selfs verleng (kvkkvk is langer as kvkvk) word, dui daarop dat die sprekers wel die uiting fonologies en morfologies kan beplan en dus nie 'n probleem het op hierdie vlak van beplanning nie. die herbeplande uitings voldoen ook aan die fonotaktiese reels van afrikaans en engels. veranderinge in die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting moet dus nie sonder meer as 'n afwyking in linguisties-simboliese beplanning beskou word nie. dit is wel moontlik dat sodanige simptome so 'n afwyking kan weerspieel, maar binne die konteks van al die gedragspatrone van die sprekers, is so 'n verklaring minder aanvaarbaar. daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat hierdie sprekers verstaanbaar verbaal kan kommunikeer en dus die uitings linguisties kan beplan. die produksie van die uitings is egter stadig en met hoorbare distorsie van die klankeienskappe. die klankstruktuur van s5 is in die meeste gevalle verkort deur byvoorbeeld die weglating van 'n lettergreep (dit het die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 a van der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots en r j grimbeek pointe en johns (1975) het ook klankvolgordefoute waargeneem. preposisionering van klanke in 'n woord is in beide studies gevind. hierdie navorsers en ook trost en canter (1974) vind dat metatese feitlik nooit voorkom nie. dit bevestig die huidige resultate want slegs een geval hiervan is waargeneem (kyk tabel 2 — omruiling van klanke). hulle bevind ook dat slegs 'n klein persentasie van die foute van verbaal apraktiese sprekers, klankvolgordefoute is. die data van hierdie ondersoek is nie op presies dieselfde wyse en met dieselfde doel ontleed as die data van la pointe en johns (1975) nie en verdere vergelykings is nie sonder verdere ontleding van die versamelde data moontlik nie. hierdie resultate kom dus daarop neer dat die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting wel 'n invloed het op frekwensie en tipe veranderinge in die struktuur van die uiting. die grootste getal veranderinge in klankstruktuur het voorgekom by s3 en s5. enkele van die simptome soos die verlenging van die struktuur van s5 kan as 'n probleem in fonologiese beplanning beskou word. die ander simptome soos die sistematiese vereenvoudiging van s3en s5-eenhede dui daarop dat die sprekers wel in staat is tot fonologiese herbeplanning van die uiting en kennis het van fonotaktiese reels. die verbaal apraktiese sprekers ondervind moontlik 'n probleem in algemene motoriese beplanning en sistematiese vooruitvoering en verander die struktuur dan na 'n vorm wat makliker geproduseer kan word. hierdie vorm stem ooreen met die sogenaamde eenheid van spraakprogrammering (volgens die meeste teoretici) naamlik die kv-lettergreep of 'n duplisering daarvan. distorsie die voorkoms van die verskillende vorms van distorsie by die verskillende klankstrukture word aangegee in tabel 3. distorsie het voorgekom by al die klankstrukture en die persentasie van voorkoms verskil nie baie nie. die hoogste persentasie is 26,6% by si en die laagste is 14,3% by s4. dit wil dus voorkom asof die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting nie 'n groot rol speel by die voorkoms van distorsie nie. distorsie is waarskynlik 'n kenmerkende simptoom van verbale apraksie wat voorkom ongeag die eienskappe van die uiting. aangeteken. 'n klankvervanging is ook aangeteken indien 'n klank vervang is met 'n klank wat artikulatories on verwant is daaraan. die onderskeid tussen distorsie en klankvervangings is egter soms 'n probleem. geen vorige navorsing is hieroor gedoen wat as riglyn gebruik kan word nie. elektromiografiese, spektrografiese en ander metodes wat spraakbewegings kan ontleed, moet in die toekoms gebruik word om distorsie by verbale apraksie na te vors. die vorm van distorsie met die hoogste. voorkoms is versteuring van stemhebbendheid wat meebring dat 'n klank nie met sekerheid herken word as stemhebbend of stemloos nie. hierdie resultaat van die ouditiewe analise bevestig dat die onvermoe tot interartikulator-sinchronisasie (tussen die glottis en artikulasiestrukture) vir korrekte stemgewing een van die prominentste en kenmerkendste probleme van die verbaal apraktiese spreker is. die syfers in tabel 3 toon aan dat meer foute in stemhebbendheid van klanke by si, s4 en s2 voorkom as by die ander strukture wat andersins meer probleme tot gevolg gehad het. die moontlikheid bestaan dat die sprekers poog om stemhebbendheid te beheer en dat die poging toeneem in intensiteit by die moeiliker en langer uitings. dit bring dan mee dat minder sat-foute voorkom by die langer en moeiliker klankstrukture. ander vorms van konsonantdistorsie het ook voorgekom en het 20,3% van die distorsiefoute uitgemaak. die meeste van hierdie foute het voorgekom by s3 en s5. die langer uitings bevat meer klanke wat foutief geproduseer kan word, maar dit het ook die /f/-klank bevat wat baie van die foute tot gevolg gehad het. vorige navorsers het ook bevind dat frikatiewe moeilik is vir verbaal apraktiese sprekers (shankweiler en harris, 1966; la pointe en johns, 1975; trost en canter, 1974; wertz et al. 1984: 52). dit is dus 'n kombinasie van faktore wat meebring dat meer konsonantdistorsies voorkom by s3 en s5. dit is interessant dat s3 ook in hierdie geval meer foute meebring as s5. die lengte van die eenheid is dus ook met betrekking tot die getal konsonantdistorsies nie die enigste bepalende faktor nie. ϊ tabel 3: distorsie by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe: distorsiefoute klankstruktuurgroepe stemhebbendheid konsonant — ander vokaal verandering van vokaaleienskappe lettergreep of eenheid j 1 totaal j 51 52 53 54 55 127 55 44 64 35 7 18 67 12 52 62 50 48 32 64 9 4 5 , 2 ' 2 0 1 5 0 4 205 (26,6%) ' 128 (16,7%) ' 169 (22%) 110 (14,3%) 157 (20,4%) totaal 325 (42,2%) 156 (20,3%) 256 (33,3%) 22 (2,9%) 10 (1,3%) 769 (100%)' die distorsies wat waargeneem is tydens die ondersoek het onkonstant voorgekom. 'n besondere klank of meer klanke binne 'n uiting is soms tydens die herhaalde produksies foutief geproduseer. die mate van distorsie het ook gevarieer. die verandering in die klankeienskappe was soms net waarneembaar terwyl dit by 'n volgende herhaling soms in so 'n mate afgewyk het dat dit byna soos 'n klankvervanging! geklink het. in sommige gevalle, wanneer die klank suiwer en vlot geproduseer is, is klankvervangings wel as sodanig distorsie van vokale het 33,3% van al die distorsiefoute uitgemaak. dit het by al die klankstrukture voorgekom. die laer voorkoms van vokaaldistorsie teenoor konsonantdistorsie bevestig die resultate van shankweiler en harris (1966). distorsie is onderskei van veranderinge in die vokaaleienskappe (2,9%) wat in werklikheid 'n'vorm van diftongisering was. in laasgenoemde geval is daar moontlik 'n onvermoe om die kritiese artikulatoriese konfigurasie te behou of om stemgewing en/of tongbeweging te onderbreek terwyl the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 51 a van der merwe, i c uys, j μ loots en r j grimbeek daar na die posisie van die volgende klank beweeg word. die ander vorms van vokaaldistorsie is moontlik weens 'n onvermoe om die kritiese ruimtelike konfigurasie te bereik. distorsie van 'n lettergreep en eenheid het slegs 1,3% van die totale aantal distorsies uitgemaak en was met 'n enkele uitsondering beperk tot s3 en s5. distorsie van enkele klanke binne 'n uiting is dus die meer algemene verskynsel by verbale apraksie. 'n teorie wat die verskynsel van distorsie by verbale apraksie verklaar, moet ook die feit verklaar dat slegs enkele klanke binne 'n uiting aangetas word, dat hierdie aantasting onkonstant plaasvind en dat meer distorsie voorkom by uitings met 'n besondere klankstruktuur. die mees algemene oorsaak vir klankdistorsie is spiertonusen spierkoordinasieprobleme soos wat voorkom by disartrie (darley et al. 1975; itoh en sasanuma, 1984). soortgelyke probleme is nie waargeneem by verbaal apraktiese sprekers nie (shankweiler en harris, 1966) en kan dus nie die voorkoms van distorsie verklaar nie. die moontlikheid dat klankdistorsie by verbale apraksie deur een of ander vorm van 'n perseptuele probleem veroorsaak word, behoort ook oorweeg te word. dit is egter nie duidelik hoe 'n ouditiewe of oraal-sensoriese terugvoeringsen/of persepsieprobleem of selfs 'n interne terugvoeringsprobleem tot onkonstante distorsie van klanke kan aanleiding gee nie. dit is meer waarskynlik dat 'n perseptuele en terugvoeringsprobleem konstant teenwoordig sal wees ( en 'n konstante probleem veroorsaak) en nie sensitiwiteit vir kontekstuele faktore sal vertoon nie. die konteks van 'n uiting soos byvoorbeeld die klankstruktuur het wel implikasies vir die beplanning en produksie van spraak. reaksie op die konteks en in hierdie geval die voorkoms van distorsie, is dus meer waarskynlik 'n aanduiding dat distorsie voorkom weens 'n versteuring in die beplanning en/of produksie van spraak. > xdie verlies van die ouditiewe beeld van die kritiese akoes/ tiese konfigurasie van 'n klank is 'n meer aanneemlike verklaring as 'n terugvoeringsof perseptuele probleem, maar dit sou waarskynlik ook aanleiding gee tot 'n konstant teenwoordige probleem. dit behoort ook nie konteks-sensitiwiteit te vertoon nie. j itoh en sy medewerkers 1(1979, 1984) wat die eerste navorsers was wat distorsie by verbale apraksie as 'n eienskap van verbale apraksie in die literatuur beskryf het, beskou distorsie as die gevolg van temporale wansinchronisasie tussen die bewegings van verskillende artikulators. dit is wel 'n redelike verklaring vir die aspek wat hulle ondersoek het, naamlik die temporale wansinchronisasie van velere en ander artikulasiebewegings. dit lei dan tot ontoepaslike nasalering of denasalering van klanke en soms gevolglike foutiewe identifikasie van klanke. dit is egter ook moontlik dat ruimtelike faktore tot distorsie aanleiding gee. soos reeds aangedui, vereis akkurate spraakproduksie dat die kritiese ruimtelike konfigurasie by herhaling bereik moet word en dat die natuurlike mate van ekwivalensie (hughes & abbs, 1976) nie oorskry word nie. sekere klanke is ruimtelik baie naby verwant en die oorskryding van die kritiese ruimtelike ekwivalensiegrense kan meebring dat daar distorsie van die klank is of selfs dat dit as 'n ander klank waargeneem word. tydens die ondersoek is waargeneem dat die mate van distorsie voortdurend wissel. die kritiese grense van ekwivalensie is dus telkens in wisselende mate oorskry. hierdie grense is dus te plasties by die verbaal apraktiese spreker. opsommend kom die resultate daarop neer dat die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting nie 'n groot rol speel by die voorkoms van distorsie nie. die vorm van distorsie met die hoogste voorkoms is versteurings van stemhebbendheid. minder vokaaldistorsies as konsonantdistorsies is waargeneem. distorsie kom onkonstant by herhaling van dieselfde uiting voor. die mate van distorsie varieer ook. hierdie feite en ook die feit dat konteks-sensitiwiteit wel voorkom, lei tot die afleiding dat distorsie voorkom weens 'n probleem in die beplanning van die temporale en ruimtelike dimensies van beweging. die kritiese ekwivalensiegrense word dan telkens in wisselende mate oorskry. afwykings in temporale vloei die aspekte wat geklassifiseer is as afwykings in temporale vloei het te doen met die ouditief waarneembare tempo van vooruitvoering van spraak. ongeveer dieselfde persentasie afwykings in temporale vloei is waargeneem by si, s2, s3 en s5 terwyl s4 baie minder foute vertoon (kyk tabel 4). in die produksie van s4 is die vokale, soos reeds met die akoestiese analises bevind, selde en minimaal verleng en afbakening in lettergrepe is minder waarskynlik. dit is interessant dat s5 minder foute opgelewer het as si, s2 en s3. daar is waargeneem dat die proefpersone die eenhede in s5 ritmies geproduseer het en moontlik het die inherente ritme van hierdie klankstruktuur temporale vloei gefasiliteer. afwykings in temporale vloei klankstruktuurgroepe verlenging van vokaal verlenging van konsosonant artikulasieverlenging stadige doelbewuste artikulasie lettergrepe kom afgebakend voor totaal 51 52 53 54 55 48 39 49 15 22 0 7 22 22 1 32 32 32 21 32 6 4 0 4 8 127 124 120 2 123 213 (23,9%) 206 (23,1%) 223 (25%) 64 (7,1%) 186 (20,9%) totaal 173 (19,4%) 52 (5,8%) 149 (16,7%) 22 (2,5%) 496 (55,6%) 892 (100%) tabel 4: afwykings in temporale vloei by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie: aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking 52 die verlenging van vokale het 19,4% van die totale getal afwykings in temporale vloei uitgemaak in teenstelling met 5,8% verlengings van konsonante. volgens crompton (1980) is die duur van vokale meer veranderlik en die verbaal apraktiese sprekers was meer geneig om dit te verleng. die konsonante wat wel verleng is, is in s3 en s4. 'n nadere ondersoek van die data bring 'n baie interessante verskynsel aan die lig. die vier sprekers met verworwe verbale apraksie het almal die finale konsonant van die kvk-eenhede (s4) en die tweede konsonant in kvkvk(s3) eenhede verleng (in gevalle waar verlenging wel voorgekom het). die feit dat dieselfde verskynsel by die vier proefpersone voorkom, impliseer dat die beplanning en vooriiitvoering van hierdie klankstrukture sekere eise stel wat op 'dieselfde wyse oorkom is. die verlenging van die finale konsonant by s4 is verrassend want oor die algemeen het die minste foute by hierdie klankstruktuur voorgekom. daar moet egter in gedagte gehou word dat die eenhede agtereenvolgend geproduseer is en moontlik is die finale klank verleng terwyl die volgende uiting beplan is. die kvkvk-struktuur kan verdeel word in 'n kvken vk-gedeelte en in hierdie geval is dit ook soos by die kvk-eenhede, die tweede konsonant wat verleng is. die vraag ontstaan of dit nie die besondere klank is wat aanleiding gee tot verlenging nie. klankstrukture 2 en 5 bevat egter ook die klanke wat verleng is (/k/ en /t/) maar by hierdie strukture vind verlenging baie minder (sewe en een keer) plaas. die besondere klankstruktuur van s3 en s4 is dus die faktor wat aanleiding gee tot die verlenging van konsonante. die artikulasieverlenging wat waargeneem is, verwys in hierdie studie na verlenging van alle klanke. dit het net by proefpersoon 4 voorgekom. akoestiese analises (van der merwe, 1986) het aan die lig gebring dat die duur van die uiting in mindere mate afwyk as vokaalduur. by hierdie persoon is die oorgange tussen klanke en dus die spoed van vooruitvoering nie stadiger as by die ander proefpersone nie, maar die statiese periode van artikulasie is verleng. die verlenging het minimale sensitiwiteit vir klankstruktuur vertoon en is dus 'n konstante verskynsel wat ook blyk uit die 32 foute by si, s2, s3 en s5. hierdie simptoom is moontlik die gevolg van die besondere letsel van proefpersoon 4, naamlik in die linkerperifere parietale gebied en in die posterior-been van die capsula interna wat onder meer deel uitmaak van die afferente sensoriese bane na die talamus en van die basale ganglia (chusid, 1973: 13; 153—159). sensoriese terugvoering tydens spraak is moontlik as gevolg van die letsel onvoldoende en die persoon kompenseer deur segmented duur te verleng. so 'n verklaring is aanvaarbaar indien aanvaar word dat sensoriese terugvoering nodig is vir spraakvorming of vir die beheer van spraak onder buitengewone omstandighede soos 'n programmeringsprobleem. stadige doelbewuste artikulasie het voorgekom by proefpersone 2 en 3 en is slegs 22 keer waargeneem. stadige doelbewuste artikulasie is 'n subjektiewe waarneming deur 'n luisteraar in teenstelling met die ander foute wat ouditief duidelik identifiseerbaar is. om 'η werklik akkurate beskrywing daarvan te gee en om die verskil hiertussen en artikulasieverlenging te bepaal, sal 'n spektrografiese ontleding van groot waarde wees. proefpersoon 3 het ook afwykings in inisiering vertoon en dit is moontlik dat die stadige doelbewuste artikulasie 'n minder ernstige simptoom is van 'n inisieringsafwyking wat daartoe lei dat die spoed van vooruitvoering stadig is. dit kan ook 'n kompensatoriese reaksie wees op die algemene probleem in programmering. die grootste afwyking in temporale vloei is die afbakening van lettergrepe en dit het by al die proefpersone voorgekom en by al die eenhede met meer as een lettergreep. dit is wel twee keer waargeneem by s4 waar proefpersoon 1 die eenheid tydens twee van die vier herhalings geproduseer het as (du k/. die afbakening van lettergrepe is ook deur vorige navorsers (kent en rosenbek, 1982 en 1983; trost en canter, 1974) waargeneem. kent en rosenbek (1982) spreek die mening uit dat hierdie simptoom 'n aanduiding is dat die beplanning van spraak in onafhanklike lettergreep-eenhede plaasvind omdat die spreker nie groter eenhede kan hanteer nie. lettergreepvooruitvoering vind dus plaas. dit is 'n logiese verklaring vir die simptoom, maar die vraag ontstaan of die eenhede werklik onafhanklik beplan is en of slegs die vooruitvoering en produksie in lettergreep-eenhede plaasvind. deur die voorkoms van koartikulasie en adaptasie by die klankomgewing na te gaan, sal moontlik 'n antwoord op hierdie vraag verkry word. hierdie resultate dui dus daarop dat verskillende vorms van afwykings in temporale vloei voorkom en dat die njeeste hiervan nie sensitiwiteit vir die klankstruktuur van die uiting vertoon nie. die verlenging van konsonante het wel meer probleme met die vooruitvoering van s3 en s4 uitgelig. die het net by die sprekers met verworwe verbale apraksie voorgekom. die resultate toon aan dat sekere simptome (soos artikulasieverlenging) meer individueel is terwyl ander (soos die afbakening van lettegrepe en vokaalverlehging) by al die sprekers voorkom. kent en rosenbek (1983) het ook individuele variasie in die vorms van temporale vkrsteuring waargeneem. die algemene indruk is dat die waarneembare simptome waarskynlik kompensatoriese aksies in reaksie op 'n onderliggende probleem in die motoriese programmering van spraak is. j i afwykings in vlotheid i probleme om spraak te inisieer word as een van die kentabel 5: afwykings in vlotheid by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe klankstruktuurgroepe 51 52 53 54 55 totaal onvlot herhaling 17 (14, vassteking (bv. glottale afsluiting) 4 5 9 2 11 afwykings in vlotheid 31 (26,3%) vassteking met hoorbare worstelgedrag 25 (21, stadige worstelende artikulasie 15 9 45 (38,1 totaal 12 (10,1%) 23 (19,5%) 27 (22,ί ' 19 (16,1 37 (31,4 118 (100%) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie: aanduidings van die aard van die afwyking 53 merkendste eienskappe van verworwe verbale apraksie beskryf (wertz et al. 1984; kent en rosenbek, 1983; collins et al. 1983). volledige simptoombeskrywings word egter nie verskaf nie. wertz et al. (1984: 76) se wel dat probleme in inisiering gekenmerk word deur "false starts and restarts". in die huidige ondersoek is vier tipes simptome waargeneem wat geklassifiseer kan word as afwykings in vlotheid (kyk tabel 5). die simptome kan ook beskryf word as afwykings in inisiering. stadige worstelende artikulasie wat die grootste persentasie (38,1%) van die totale aantal afwykings in vlotheid uitmaak, het slegs by proefpersoon 3 voorgekom. die meeste van die foute in die ander drie subkategoriee is ook gemaak deur proefpersoon 3. die afwykings in vlotheid het slegs 4,7% van die totale aantal foute uitgemaak en is skynbaar nie 'n tipiese probleem van die groep verbaal apraktiese sprekers nie. dit is moontlik dat hierdie sprekers anders sal reageer in spontane spraak. die meeste afwykings in vlotheid het voorgekom by s3 en s5. rosenbek (1980) se ook dat meer afwykings in vlotheid by langer eenhede voorkom. 'n interessante verskynsel is dat proefpersoon 3 meer gevalle van stadige worstelende artikulasie by s4 as by die ander klankstrukture vertoon het. die data is nagegaan vir enige tendense wat hierdie verskynsel kan verklaar. die enigste moontlike verklaring is dat die vooruitvoering van die kvk-struktuur moontlik moeilik is omdat dit nie die natuurlike eenheid van programmering is nie. daar is verskillende moontlike oorsake vir die herhalings, vasstekings en worstelende artikulasie wat by verbale apraksie voorkom. daar kan 'n onvermoe wees om die bewegings temporaal te orden en vooruit te voer of sensoriese terugvoering en interne terugvoering kan moontlik swak gekoordineer word met programmering. dit kan ook 'n ware inisieringsprobleem wees en die spreker is dan nie in staat om te begin beweeg nie. laasgenoemde word soms beskryf as 'n ideomotoriese apraksie (rosenbek, 1980). 1 . opsommend kom die resultate daarop neer dat daar aandui7 dings is dat vlotheid en inisiering wel in 'n mate kontekssensitief is. meer afwykings in vlotheid het by die langer uitings, naamlik s3 en s5,' voorgekom en proefpersoon 3 het meer stadige worstelende artikulasie by die kvk-struktuur vertoon. die verbaal apraktiese sprekers vertoon nie in dieselfde mate afwykings irl vlotheid nie. ι spraakfoute gevolg deur selfkorreksie spraakfoute gevolg deur selfkorreksie het meer by s5 en s3 as by die ander klankstrukture voorgekom (kyk tabel 6) en dit was teenwoordig by al die proefpersone/behalwe by proefpersoon 1. dit was slegs verbetering van klankseleksiefoute wat voorgekom het en proefpersoon 1 het baie min hiervan vertoon. ongeveer dieselfde getal klankveranderinge en lettergreepveranderinge het voorgekom. in die geval van klankveranderinge was dit meestal suksesvol en by lettergreepveranderinge meestal onsuksesvol. die feit dat klanken lettergreepveranderinge voorgekom het, dui daarop dat die verbaal apraktiese sprekers bewus is van foutiewe klankseleksie en poog om dit te verbeter. dit was ook die geval by proefpersoon 5. dit is opvallend dat geen ander vorms van selfkorreksie, byvoorbeeld van distorsies, voorgekom het nie. die sprekers is dus nie bewus van die groot getal distorsiefoute nie of is bewus van hul onvermoe om dit te verbeter. in 'n sekere sin bevestig hierdie bevindinge die standpunt van itoh en sasanuma (1984) dat distorsie die kernsimptoom van verbale apraksie is. selfkorreksie by verbaal apraktiese sprekers is nog nie formeel ondersoek nie en net beperkte vergelykings met ander studies kan getref word. deal en darley (1972) het wel die vermoe om foute te voorspel by verbaal apraktiese sprekers nagegaan en gevind dat hulle daartoe in staat is, maar dat meer foute gemaak word as wat voorspel is. die proefpersone was ook in staat om sommige van hul foute te herken. afwykings in prosodie afwykings in prosodie verwys in hierdie studie net na simptome soos oormatige of afwykende klemen intonasiepatrone en momentele afwykende stemkwaliteit. afwykings in prosodie is ook deur ander navorsers waargeneem (kent en rosenbek; 1982, 1983). dit word beskryf as disprosodie en verwys in besonder na stadige spraakspoed, verlengde segmentele duur en lettergreep-afbakening. hierdie aspekte is in hierdie ondersoek afsonderlik bestudeer. tabel 7: afwykings in prosodie by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe klankstruktuurgroep totaal si s2 s3 s4 s5 totaal aantal foute 53 (21,9%) 57 (23,6%) 48 (19,8%) 46 (19%) 38 (15,7%) 242 (100%) klankstruktuurgroepe spraakfoute gevolg deur selfkorreksie klankstruktuurgroepe klankverandering lettergreepverandering totaal klankstruktuurgroepe suksesvol onsuksesvol suksesvol onsuksesvol totaal si 1 0 1 2 4 (12,5%) s2 2 1 1 1 5 (15,6%) s3 3 0 2 3 8 (25%) s4 0 0 1 0 1 (3,1%) s5 6 2 2 4 14 (43,8%) totaal ' 12 (37,5%) 3 (9,4%) 7 (21,9%) 10 (31,2%) 32 (100%) tabel 6: spraakfoute gevolg deur selfkorreksie by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 a v a n d e r m e r w e , i c u y s , j μ l o o t s e n r j g r i m b e e k die prosodiese aspekte wat in hierdie studie nagegaan is, is nog slegs in beperkte mate deur andere ondersoek. kent en rosenbek (1983) het wel op grond van spektrografiese analises waargeneem dat daar min variasie in piekintensiteit oor 'n aantal lettergrepe by verbaal apraktiese sprekers is. die beoordeling van die aspek in hierdie studie was nie in soveel diepte nie. daar is wel waargeneem dat die afwyking in prosodie dikwels vererger het wanneer die proefpersoon die uiting by herhaling korrek kon se. prosodiefoute het, afgesien van 'n enkele uitsondering by proefpersoon 1, slegs by proefpersoon 2, 3 en 4 voorgekom en glad nie by proefpersoon 5 nie. die afwykings in prosodie het by al die klankstrukture voorgekom (kyk tabel 7). dit is dus 'n konstant teenwoordige probleem wat nie konteks-sensitief is nie. dit wil voorkom asof alle afwykings in prosodie nie by alle verbaal apraktiese sprekers voorkom nie. hierdie probleme moet egter met akoestiese tegnieke ondersoek word alvorens enige betroubare afleidings gemaak kan word. gevolgtrekkings die resultate van hierdie ondersoek dui daarop dat verbale apraksie 'n afwyking is wat gekenmerk word deur: — distorsie van klanke wat soms soos vervangings van klanke klink; — afwykings in temporale vloei met veral die afbakening van lettergrepe en verlenging van klanke; —/veranderinge in die struktuur van 'n eenheid met veral 'n neiging om dit te vereenvoudig na 'n kv-eenheid of duplisering daarvan; — vervangings met eenheidsklanke van veral konsonante; —/afwykings in die vlotheid van spraak; -4 pogings tot selfkorreksie van klankvervangings; en τafwykings in die prosodiese eienskappe van spraak; al die simptome kom nie in dieselfde mate by al die-verbaal apraktiese sprekers voor nie. 'n verdeling van die simptome op grond van konteks-sensitiwiteit dui daarop dat distorsie van klanke, lettergreepafbakening en ook onkonstantheid in produksie, kernprobleme van verbale apraksie weerspieel. vervangings, byvoegings en weglatings wat konteks-sensitief is, is moontlik geassosieerde simptome en kompensatoriese strategiee om die kompleksiteit van 'n uiting te verminder. dit wil voorkom asof die verbaal apraktiese spreker 'n onderliggende onvernioe het om konstant die kritiese temporale en ruimtelike ewivalensiegrense van spraakbewegings te bereik, spraakklanke viiinig en onveranderlik vooruit te voer en om spraakuitings—vooruit te beplan. die verbaal apraktiese spreker is wel bewus van sy probleem en poog om die spraakuitings te vereenvoudig. die resultate van hierdie ondersoek dui verder ook daarop dat verbale apraksie voorkom na verworwe breinskade, maar dat 'n aangebore onvermoe om spraak te beplan, wel ook kan bestaan. erkennings geldelike bystand van die raad van geesteswetenskaplike navorsing vir hierdie ondersoek, word hiermee erken. verwysings aten, j.l., darley, f.l., deal, j.l. en johns, d.f. comment on a.d. martin's "some objections to the term apraxia of speech". journal of speech and hearing disorders, 40, 416—420, 1975. buckingham, h.w. explanation of apraxia with consequences for the concept of apraxia of speech. brain and language, 8, 2 0 2 2 2 6 , 1979. chusid, j.g. correlative neuroanatomy and fanctional neurology, 15de uitgawe, los altos, california: lange medical publications, 1973. collins, m., rosenbek, j.c. en wertz, r.t. spectrographic analysis of vowel and word duration. journal of speech and hearing research, 26, 2 2 4 2 3 0 , 1983. crompton, a. timing patterns in french. phonetica, 37, 205—234, 1980. darley, f.l., aronson, a. en brown, j. motor speech disorders. philadelphia: saunders, 1974. deal, j.l. en darley, f.l. the influence of linguistic and situational variables on phonemic accuracy in apraxia of speech .journal of speech and hearing research, 15, 639—653, 1972. dunlop, j.m. en marquardt, t.p. linguistic and articulatory aspects of single word production in apraxia of speech. cortex, 13, 17-29, 1977. edwards, m.l. en shriberg, l.d. phonology: applications in communicative disorders. california: college-hill press, 1983. guyard, h., sabouraud, o. & gagnepain, j. a procedure to differentiate phonological disturbances in broca's aphasia and wernicke's aphasia. brain and language, 13, 19—30, 1981. hughes, o.m. en abbs, j.h. labial-mandibular coordination in the production of speech: implications for the operation of motor equivalence. phonetica, 33, 199—221, 1976. itoh, m., sasanuma, s. en ushijima, t. velar movements during speech in a patient with apraxia of speech. brain and language. 7, 227-239, 1979. itoh, m. en sasanuma, s. articulatory movements in apraxia of speech. in j.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil en a.e. aronson |reds.| apraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics, management. california: college-hill press, 1984'. jakobson, r., fant, c.g.m. en halle, m. preliminaries to speech analysis. massachusets: mit press, 1967. jakobson, r. studies on child language and aphasia. paris: mouton, 1971. , ^ johns, d.f. en darley, f.l. phonemic variability in apraxia of speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 13, 556—583, 1970. keller, e. parameters for vowel substitutions in broca's aphasia. brain and language, 5, 265—285, 1978. kent, r.d. en rosenbek, j.c. prosodie disturbance and neurologic lesion. brain and language, 15, 259—291, 1982. kent, r.d. en rosenbek, j.c. accoustic patterns of apraxia1 of speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 26, 231—249, 1983. ' • la pointe, l.l. en johns, d.f. some phonemic characteristics i in apraxia of speech. journal of communication disorders, .8, 2 5 9 2 6 9 , 1975. i la pointe, l.l. apraxia of speech and its management. ongepubliseerde referaat voorgedra by die agste jaarlikste kursus in gedragsneurologie en neuropsigologie, florida, v.s.a., 1982! martin, a.d. some objections to the term apraxia of speech. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 39, 53—64, 1974. \ " , miceli, g., gainotti, g., caltagirone, c. en masullo, c. some aspects of phonological impairment in aphasia. brain and language, 11, 159-169, 1980. rosenbek, j.c. apraxia of speech — relationship to stuttering. journal of fluency disorders, 5, 233—254, 1980. shankweiler, d.p. en harris, k.s. an experimental approach to the problem of articulation in aphasia. cortex, 2, 277—297, 1966. trost, j.e. en canter, g.j. apraxia of speech in patients with broca's aphasia: a study of phoneme_production accuracy_ and error patterns. brain and language, 1, 63—79, 1974. van der merwe, a. die motoriese beplanning van spraak/by verbale apraksie. ongepubliseerde d.phil-verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria, 1986. van der merwe, α., uys, i.c., loots, j.m. en grimbeek, r.j. die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie.' die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, 34, 10—22, 1987. wertz, r.t., la pointe, l.l. en rosenbek, j.c. apraxia of speech in adults: the disorder and its management. orlando: grune & stratton, inc., 1984. \ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e comprehension o f deictic terms in normal a n d l a n g u a g e i m p a i r e d children michelle mentis, b . a . ( s p . & η . t h : ) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) department speech therapy and audiology, groote schuur hospital, cape town summary this study examined the comprehension of four pairs of deictic terms in a group of language impaired children and compared their interpretation of these terms with those of non-language impaired children of the same age range. each group was comprised of ten subjects within the age range of 9,6 to 10,6 years. two tasks were administered, one to assess the comprehension of the terms here, there, this, and that and the other to assess the comprehension of the terms, come, go, bring and take. the results showed that while the non-language impaired subjects comprehended the full deictic contrast between the pairs of terms tested, the language impaired group did not. a qualitative analysis of the data revealed that the language impaired subjects appeared to follow the same developmental sequence as normal children in their acquisition of these terms and responded by using the same strategies that younger non-language impaired children use at equivalent stages of development. furthermore, the language impaired subjects appeared to comprehend the deictic terms in a predictable order based on their relative semantic complexity. opsomming hierdie studie ondersoek die verstaanbaarheid van vier pare diektiese terme in 'n groep taalversteurde kinders en vergelyk hulle interpretasie van hierdie terme met die van normale kinders in dieselfde ouderdomsgroep. elke groep is saamgestel uit tien proefpersone in die ouderdomsgroep van 9,6 tot 10,6 jaar. twee take is aan hulle gestel, om hulle begrip van die terme: hier, daar, hierdie en daardie en kom, gaan, bring en neem/vat vas te stel. in die eerste taak is van die kinders verwag om volgens instruksie een van 'n diere paar te beweeg, en in die tweede, moes hulle die "spreker" en die "aangespreekte" uitken. resultate het aangetoon dat terwyl die normale kinders die diektiese kontras tussen die paar terme wat getoets is, verstaan, die taalversteurde kinders, dit nie begryp het nie. 'n kwalitatiewe analise van die data het aangedui dat die taalversteurde kinders skynbaar dieselfde ontwikkelings-volgorde, as dii van normale kinders in die aanleer van hierdie terme volg, en dat van dieselfde strategiee as die van jonger normale kinders met 'n ooreenstemmende ontwikkelingsvlak, gebruikgemaak is. dit het geblyk dat die taalversteurde kinders die diektiese terme in 'n voorspelbare volgorde, gebaseer op die terme se relatiewe semantiese kompleksiteit, verstaan. deixis has been defined as "the term for linguistic devices that anchor the utterance to the communicative setting in which it occurs". 1 4 deictic terms are used to point out a particular object, position or direction in relation to the speaker, and can only be interpreted in the actual context of such utterances, with reference to the specific characteristics of the spatio-temporal conditions which exist at the moment of utterance. 1 1 in fact, ingram, as cited by r e e s , 1 4 states that "deictic features handle the fact that language is used to communicate between speakers and hearers". as such, it is intimately bound to the area of pragmatics which occupies the interface between linguistic, cognitive and social development. 1 4 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comprehension of deictic terms 93 traditionally deixis has been divided into three categories: time, person and place deixis. place deixis, the concern of the present study comprises devices for expressing the speaker's view of his position relative to the listener and other surroundings.1 4 examples of these are the locational adverbs here and there, the demonstrative adjectives this and that, and the verbs come, go, bring and take. there are three factors that contribute to the underlying complexity of deictic terms. 4 ' 6 ' 1 3 these are that: deictic terms all have a point of reference; they involve a shifting reference; and they have shifting boundaries. the normal point of reference for deictic terms is the speaker, e.g. here locates the place near the speaker as opposed to the place further away (there) and this locates an object near the speaker and that an object away from the speaker. here and this are thus proximal with respect to the point of reference, while there and that are nonproximal. from this it can be seen that the perception of deictic contrasts involves the mastery of two principles: the speaker principle which indicates that the speaker is the point of reference; and the distance principle which indicates that each pair of terms contrasts on the distance dimension.6 the fact that all deictic expressions involve a shifting reference indicates that the meaning of these terms varies depending on who is speaking at that particular time. it is because all deictic terms involve shifting reference that the speaker principle is difficult to master. 6 however, the reference of here and there not only shifts with every change of speaker, but in addition to this shifts with every change of position of each speaker. here locates the speaker's place which may or may not include the listener's place. if it does not, then the location of there also shifts with every change of listener. thus there shifts reference even more than here. the more shifting a term involves, the harder it is for the child to master. thus as the non-proximal terms (there and that) shift more than the proximal ones, they are more difficult to acquire.4' 6 ' 7 furthermore, it has been suggested that the difference in the amount of shifting between the terms predisposes children to treat both terms of a pair as if they were proximal.6 the distance principle may be considered to be difficult to acquire because of the shifting boundaries of deictic terms. this refers to the fact that the physical space located by the word here differs according to the context in which it is used. thus here may be used to indicate the precise spot on the floor where the speaker is standing (here where i am) or the room he is in (here in the study). the context of discourse thus determines the distance that will be referred to. the foregoing discussion illustrates the fact that in order for spatial deictic terms to be understood, the hearer must not only know the speaker's position in space and his point of view, but must also understand the context of the utterance. furthermore, he needs to know whether his own position and point of view differs from that of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 94 michelle i the speaker. the differing levels of complexity for spatial deictic terms are represented in table i. table i: degree of complexity of spatial deictic terms deictic terms degree of complexity 1. here — there speaker and place 2. this — that speaker and place and object 3. come — go speaker and place and object and movement 4. bring — take speaker and place and object and movement and cause it has been suggested that the true deictic contrast between pairs of deictic terms is acquired through a process in which semantic features are added to the words until the adult meaning is attained. 1 6 this is in accord with clark's 2 , 3 · 9 semantic feature hypothesis (sfh). the acquisition of the deictic verbs come/go and take/bring has been found to be compatible with that predicted on the basis of the relative semantic complexity of the four deictic terms. 5 however, recent studies indicate that the positive, marked terms here and this are acquired earlier than their negative, unmarked counterparts there and that. 6 · 1 6 this result shows that semantic markedness is not applicable to the acquisition of polarity for these terms. this discrepancy has been explained in terms of the fact that children rely on non-linguistic strategies in interpreting the meanings of words and that for the deictic pairs herelthere and this/that, the non-linguistic strategy coincided with the negative marked member of the pair, and not, as is usually found, with the positive, unmarked member. 5 ' 6 ' 1 6 in the course of working out the contrast between deictic terms, children apply different strategies which are the outcome of their changing hypotheses about the meanings of the words. the acquisition of such contrasts has been shown to be characterized by three stages: no contrast, where the children usually use only one of the deictic pair to represent both situations; partial contrast where the other word in the deictic pair is used but only partially or incorrectly contrasted to its counterpart, and then finally the full contrast where the true deictic contrast is perceived.4' 5 ' 6 to the present writer's knowledge, no research has been undertaken to date on deictic comprehension in language-impaired children. since, as demonstrated above, deixis appears to be a rich area for characterizing the pragmatic capabilities of the child and for examining the different stages of acquisition, the potential contribution to the study of language impairment is felt to be great. in particular, this aspect may the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comprehension of deictic terms 95 be sensitive to the language difficulties of the older child whose problems so often manifest at the level of pragmatics. the present study was thus undertaken to compare the comprehension of deictic expressions in normal and language-impaired children. m e t h o d o l o g y aim to examine the comprehension of deictic terms in a group of languageimpaired children and compare their interpretation of these terms with those of non-language impaired children of the same age range. for the purposes of this study the following contrasting pairs of place deixis were chosen: here — there, which differentiate between the place where the speaker is as opposed to where he is not; this — that, which differentiate between something in the same location of the speaker and something in a different location; come — go, which differentiate between the movement of a person or object towards or away from the place where the speaker is, was or expects to be; and bring — take, which are the causative counterparts of the terms come — go. hypotheses 1. whereas the non-language impaired children will comprehend the full deictic contrast between the pairs of terms, the language impaired children will not. 2. although the language impaired children will be delayed in their comprehension of these terms, they will employ the same series of strategies in dealing with the tasks as the non-language impaired children. 3. the language impaired children will comprehend these terms in a predictable order based on the semantic complexity of the terms and according to acquisition in non-language impaired children.5' ' subjects two groups of subjects (ss) comprising an experimental (e) group of ten language impaired children and a control (c) group of ten non-language impaired children were selected. on the basis of a pilot study, the age range was between 9, 6 and 10, 6 years. all ss came from white, english-speaking, middle class south african homes. they were required to be of average intelligence and to have hearing within normal limits. due to the visual nature of the task, ss were also required to have no visual spatial problems as assessed by the occupational therapists attached to the remedial schools. tasks taskl to assess the comprehension of the deictic terms here/there and this/that. (based on a study designed by clark and sengul6). testing die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) michelle mentis was carried out under two conditions: (a) with the tester and child seated at a table beside each other (same perspective) and (b) with the tester and child seated at a table opposite each other (different perspective). child tester figure la: testing situation used for task 1: condition a tester * — 62cm 15cm j — η ο o 15cm 62cm child figure lb: testing situation used for task 1: condition b one pair of identical toy animals was placed on the table for each instruction. the task was introduced as a game in which the s had to decide which of two toy animals moved. on each trial the tester placed a pair of animals on the table, named them alternately and asked the child to look at them both. instructions such as "make the dog over there turn around" or "make this chicken hop" were then given and the s was required to respond by making one of the animals move. there were eight practice trials, one for each combination of word and condition followed by thirty-two test trials. ' task 2 to assess the comprehension of the deictic terms come/go and bring/take. (based on a study designed by clark and garnica5). each the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comprehension of deictic terms 97 child was seated next to the tester at the table with a display board on which was placed a model house, pool, barn and garden. (these models will be referred to as goals.) for each instruction, one set of three animals was arranged on the board as if at the points of a roughly equilateral triangle with one point inside one of the goals and the other two outside as shown in figure 2. the three animals on the points all faced each other. with bring and take the appropriate animal carried a small object. the task was introduced as a game in which various animals lived on a farm and in which all the animals could speak to each other. each child had to tell the tester which animal was talking or to whom a particular animal was talking — i.e. identify either the speaker or addressee. each of the four deictic verbs come, go, bring and take were used in each of four situations. in two of the situations the child had to identify the speaker (sp. 1 and sp. 2) and in the other two, had to identify the addressee (ad. 1 and ad. 2). on each trial the tester placed the three animals in their appropriate position, named them and the goal and then gave the child instructions such as 'which animal can say to the lion: "come into the h o u s e ? ' " or 'the pig says: "can i come into the b a r n ? ' " which animal is he talking to? the child was then required to identify either the speaker or the addressee. there were four practice trials, one for each verb in each of the four situations followed by forty-eight instructions, twenty-four with come and go and twenty-four with bring and take. a n a l y s i s o f r e s u l t s for task 1 each s's responses were scored as correct or incorrect according to which animal they moved. for task 2 each s's responses were scored as correct or incorrect according to the choice of animal as speaker or addressee. results of both tasks were analysed on an overall basis as well as on an individual task basis. the data were subjected to both statistical and qualitative analysis. results a n d discussion t a s k 1 a summary of mean scores and percentages of semantically correct responses produced by each group can be seen in table ii. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 98 michelle i table ii mean scores and percentages of semantically correct responses produced by each group on task 1. ' deictic term speaker beside speaker opposite c ε c ε x % x % x % x % here 4 100 3,8 95 4 100 1,4 35 there 4 100 2,2 55 4 100 2,5 62,5 here there 8 100 6 75 8 100 3,9 48,75 this 4 100 3,9 97,5 3,7 92,5 1,1 27,5 that 4 100 1,8 45 4 100 2,8 70 this that 8 100 5,7 71,25 7,7 96,25 3,9 48,75 table iii mean scores of semantically correct responses to proximal and distal terms in each of the two situations (task 1) deictic term speaker beside speaker opposite here — this 7,7 2,5 there — that 3,3 5,3 table iv: number of ss that fell into each category for each pair of terms (task 1) here — there this — that no contrast partial contrast full contrast no contrast partial contrast full contrast ε 4 4 1 4 4 1 c 0 0 10 0 0 / 10 all ss in group c perceived the full deictic contrast between both pairs of terms in both situations, and were thus considered to have mastered both the speaker and the distance principles. these results support the first aspect of h j . ss in group ε did not perceive the full deictic contrast between the pairs of terms tested in either situation. these results fully supported the second aspect of h j . the percentage of correct responses varied according to the specific term used and the situation in which it was the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comprehension of deictic terms 99 used. this was in accord with the prediction ( h 3 ) that the ss woul comprehend the full meaning of each term in a predictable order based on the semantic complexity of the terms and the order of acquisition in non-language impaired children. thus in the "speaker beside" situation group ε ss obtained a higher mean score for the linguistically simpler deictic pair here—there than for the pair this—that (t = 8; η = 10; ρ < 0,025) and for the linguistically simpler proximal terms (there & that) (τ = 0; ν = 10; ρ < 0,005). however, in the "speaker opposite" situation ss obtained similar scores for both pairs of deictic terms (t = 10; ν = 10; not significant) and as can be seen in table ii were more often correct for the non-proximal terms (t = 6; ν = 10; ρ < 0,025). a reason for the ss obtaining similar scores for both pairs of deictic terms may be that the task in this situation was a more complex one as it required full perception of the deictic contrast. correct responses to both pairs of terms was dependent on the ss' ability to take the speaker's perspective and was thus dependent on mastery of the speaker principle. with respect to the ss being more often correct on the non-proximal terms, it was felt that this was not a result of their comprehending the true meaning of the terms there and that but rather a result of their overextended use of the proximal terms. thus if the ss responded to both terms as if they meant closer to the child, then in the speaker opposite situation, the ss would respond incorrectly to the proximal terms but correctly to the non-proximal ones. this is then in accord with the hypothesis ( h 3 ) that the semantically simpler terms are acquired before their more complex counterparts. this phenomenon of overextension occurs when the meaning of one member of a pair of terms is extended to cover both words, and has been frequently cited as occurring during the initial stages of semantic development of relational terms and adjective pairs. 1 , 1 5 in the present study the phenomenon of overextension was evident in the responses of all ss in group ε at the most primitive level of comprehension of deictic terms i.e. the no contrast stage. this provides support for h 3 as the language impaired ss were responding in a similar way to that reported in younger non-language impaired children at an equivalent stage of semantic development. in order to analyse what strategies the ss were using in responding, the data for each child was studied qualitatively. it was felt that an analysis of the strategies the ss used in interpreting the words would provide the writer with insight into what hypotheses they had formed about the meaning of the words. the ss were calssified into groups on the basis of their individual response patterns in the manner adopted by clark and sengul.6 they divided the response patterns they obtained into three distinct categories: no contrast, partial contrast and full contrast. their results indicated that these categories were a function of the ages of their ss and that they thus represented three ordered stages of acquisition in die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 100 michelle i normal children. the ss in this study were grouped according to these stages. all group c subjects were at the full contrast stage whereas only one s in group ε perceived the full deictic contrast. four of the ss in group ε were at the no contrast stage, and the other four ss were at the partial contrast stage. these results support h j (see table iv). a consideration of these patterns and strategies indicated that the ss followed two basic routes in comprehending the full deictic contrast. those ss taking the child-centered route relied on the strategy of choosing the animal nearest themselves and those ss taking the speaker centered route relied on the strategy of choosing the animal nearest the speaker. ss in group ε who appeared to be functioning at the no contrast , and partial contrast stages appeared to use the strategies younger nonlanguage impaired children use in their acquisition of deictic terms.6· 1 6 this supports h 2 and is consistent with what has been previously reported on the language development of language-impaired children. it has been shown that the behaviour of language-impaired children is rule governed and that although delayed, follows the same development sequence of normal children. 1 0 ' 1 2 in further support of this prediction, it appeared that nine ss in group ε were developing comprehension of the full deictic contrast through stages in which semantic features were added in a hierarchical order in accord with clark's s m h . 3 this is consistent with what has been reported on the semantic development in non-language impaired children.2' 8 task 2 a summary of mean scores and percentages of correct responses produced by each group can be seen in table v. table v : mean scores and percentages of correct responses produced by each group (task 2) come go come go bring take bring take x % x % x % x % x % x % c 12 100 11,2 93,3 23,2 96,6 11,5 95,8 11,9 99,2 23,4 97,5 ε 10,5 87,5 3,2 26,6 13,7 57,1 10,1 84,1 4 33,314,1 58,75 whereas all ss in group c perceived the full deictic contrast between both pairs of terms, ss in group ε did not. these results support hi. the percentage of correct responses in group ε varied according to the specific term used. although it was postulated that the semantically simpler verb pair come—go would be acquired before its more complex causative counterpart bring—take ( h 3 ) , this view was not supported (t = 21; ν = 10; not significant). this result may be the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comprehension of deictic terms t a b l e vi: number of ss that fell into each category for each pair of terms (task 2) come — go bring — tak 0 no contrast partial contrast full contrast no contrast partial contrast full contrast ε 7 2 0 5 4 0 c 0 1 9 0 0 10 explained in terms of the relative frequencies of use of the deictic verbs in the south african english dialect with the assumption that the more frequent terms are easier to learn because the child will have greater exposure to them. this would be in accordance with the view that external environmental factors influence language acquisition.1' 7 ss in group ε made significantly fewer errors on the semantically simpler positive pair come—bring than they did on the semantically more complex negative pair go—take (t = 4; ν = 10; ρ < 0,01). these results provide support for h 3 and are consistent with the claim that positive unmarked members of word pairs are acquired first.9 however, this is in contrast to the fact that the proximal pair here and this have been discussed as being linguistically simpler and acquired earlier than the non-proximal pair there and that even although the non-proximal pair represent the positive unmarked terms while the proximal pair represent their negative marked counterparts.6' 1 this may be explained in terms of the fact that the ss were responding on the basis of non-linguistic strategies (for example the proximity and egocentric bias) and these were compatible with the negative marked terms rather than with the positive unmarked ones. in the above instances however, it can be argued that the non-linguistic response biases corresponded with the positive unmarked terms. the data for each child was classified into the following groups on the basis of their correct responses: no contrast; partial contrast and full contrast these categories were based on the results obtained by clark and garnica5 who statistically revealed that the categories were age based and hypothesized that they represented the normal stages of acquisition of these terms. all group c ss fell into the full contrast category for the verbs come and go, while nine of the ten group c ss fell into the full contrast category for the verbs bring and take. the other s fell into the partial contrast category for these verbs. group ε ss fell into the no contrast and partial contrast categories. for the verbs come and go, seven ss fell into the no contrast category while two ss fell into the partial contrast category. for the verbs bring and take, five ss fell into the no contrast category while four ss fell into the partial contrast category. these results support h, (see table vi). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 102 michelle i as with task 1, ss in group ε who appeared to be functioning at the no contrast and partial contrast stages appeared to use the strategies younger non-language impaired children use in their acquisition of deictic verbs.5 this supports h 2 . conclusion results from the statistical and qualitative analysis of the data indicated that the language impaired ss were delayed in their comprehension of deictic terms in comparison with the non-language impaired ss. these results fully supported the first hypothesis. group c contained a higher overall mean score for the comprehension of all deictic terms tested (9,8) than did group ε (5,82). (u = 0; n j = n 2 = 10; ρ < 0,001). a qualitative analysis of the data revealed that although the language impaired ss were delayed in their use of these terms, they appeared to follow the same developmental sequence as normal children, passing through the stages of no contrast and partial contrast before perceiving the full deictic contrast. in support of the second hypothesis it was found that the language impaired ss primarily used the same strategies in responding that non-language impaired children use, and exhibited the developmental errors previously observed in younger normal children. 5 · 61 6 the third hypothesis predicted that the language impaired ss would comprehend the deictic terms in a predictable order based on the semantic complexity of the terms and according to acquisition in non-language impaired children. in support of this hypothesis it was found that ss in group ε obtained significantly fewer errors on a predictable member in each pair of terms. however, there was no significant difference between the comprehension of each pair of deictic terms. h 3 was thus only partially supported. the findings of the present study indicated that the language impaired children tested were having problems on a semantic level. this suggests that the current academic interest in semantics should be extended to diagnostic testing and the planning of therapy. furthermore, in view of the dearth of tests for the assessment of older language impaired children, tests involving the comprehension of deictic terms would be a useful part of diagnostic assessment. references / 1. campbell, p. and wales, p. 1(1977): the study of language acquisition, chap. 13 in new horizons in linguistics, lyons, j., (ed.), penguin books ltd., harmondsworth, england. 2. clark, ε. v. (1971): on the acquisition of the meaning of before and after. / . verb. learn. verb. beh., 10, 266-275. 3. clark, ε. v. (1973a): what's in a word? on the child's acquisition of semantics in his. first language in cognitive the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comprehension of deictic terms 103 development and the acquisition of language, moore, τ. e . , (ed.), academic press, new york. 4. clark, ε. v. (1978): from gesture to word: on the natural history of deixis in language acquisition, chap. 4 in human growth and development. wolfson college lectures 1976 bruner, j. s. and garton, a. (eds.), oxford university press, oxford. 5. clark, ε. v. and garnica, ο. k. (1974): is he coming or going? on the acquisition of deictic verbs. j. verb. learn. verb. beh., 13, 556-572. 6. clark, e. and sengul, j. (1978): strategies in the acquisition of deixis. / . child. lang., 5, 457-575. 7. de villiers, p. a. and de villiers, j. g. (1974): on this, that, and the other. nonegocentricsm in very young children. j. exp. ch. psych., 18, 438-447. 8. de villiers, p. a . and de villiers, j. g. (1978): the development of word meaning. chap. 5 in language acquisition, harvard university press, cambridge. 9. donaldson, m. and balfour, g. (1968): less is more. a study of language comprehension in children. brit. j. psychol., 59, 461-471. 10. lackner, j. r. (1976): a development study of language behaviour in retarded children. in normal and deficient child language. morehead, d. m. and morehead, a. e. (eds.), university park press. 11. macrae, a . j. (1976): movement and location in the acquisition of deictic verbs. j. child lang., 3, 191-204. 12. morehead, d . m. (1976): the development of base syntax in normal and linguistically deviant children. in normal and deficient child language. morehead, d. m. and morehead, a. e. (eds.), university park press, baltimore. 13. olson, d. r. (1977): the contexts of language acquisition. chap. 13 in language learning and thoughts. macnamara, j. (ed.), academic press, new york. 14. rees, n. s. (1978): pragmatics of language. applications to normal and disordered language development. chap. 5 in bases of language intervention. schiefelbusch, r. l. (ed.), university park press. baltimore. 15. townsend, d. j. (1976): do children interpret "marked" comparative adjectives as their opposites? / . child lang., 3, 385-396. 16. webb, p. a. and abrahamson, a. a. (1976): stages of egocentricsm in children's use of "this and that". a different point of view. / . child lang., 3, 349-367. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a i d s for • t h e d e v e l o p m e n t of p e r c e p t i o n • t h e a c q u i s i t i o n of s p e e c h a n d l a n g u a g e skills • t h e i m p r o v e m e n t of m o t o r c o o r d i n a t i o n plus • helpful t e x t s for t h e r a p i s t s • e d u c a t i o n a l toys, b o o k s a n d e q u i p m e n t • r e c o r d s for a u d i t o r y t r a i n i n g • c a t a l o g u e s o n r e q u e s t • l a r g e v a r i e t y of tests a v a i l a b l e s t o c k i s t s o f • learning to listen • t w o sound lottos • full lda range play and schoolroom 8 t y r w h i t t a v e n u e , r o s e b a n k ( a d j o i n i n g t h e c o n s t a n t i a c i n e m a ) t e l e p h o n e s : 7 8 8 1 3 0 4 p . o . b o x 52137^, s a x o n w o l d , 2 1 3 2 ο m the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language intervention at schools: changing orientations w i t h i n the s.a. context 9 language intervention at schools: changing orientations within the south african context erna alant, d phil (pretoria) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of pretoria abstract the role of the speech therapist in the school has changed drastically over the last decade. the reasons for these changes originate from a growing realisation of the importance of contextualising intervention within a particular community. this article aims at providing an analysis of the present school population in south africa with specific reference to the black schools as a basis for discussion on the role of the speech and language therapist within this context. the problems of second language learning and teaching are highlighted and the role of the language therapist as a consultant within the black school system is emphasized. opsomming die rol van die spraak en taalterapeut binne die skoolkonteks het die afgelope dekade drastiese veranderinge ondergaan. die redes vir hierdie veranderinge spruit uit 'n groeiende bewuswording van die belangrikheid van die kontekstualisering van intervensie binne 'n bepaalde gemeenskap. hierdie artikel poogom 'n analisetemaakvan diehuidigeskoolbevolkingvan suid-afrika met spesifiekeverwysingna die swart skole as basis vir bespreking van die rol van die spraak en taalterapeut binne hierdie konteks. die probleme van tweedetaalonderrig en -leer asook die rol van die taalterapeut as konsultant binne die swart skoolsisteem word beklemtoon. the role of the speech and language therapist in language remediation has changed drastically over the last decade. these changes are evident in diagnostic and therapeutic intervention and can be seen as,an outcome of the paradigm shift from a traditional medical model of service where the therapist was mainly involved in individual therapy, to a functional or consultative model where the therapist is more involved in the facilitation of the individual's communication within specific functional contexts (marvin, 1987). the n e w pragmatic approach (gallagher et al. 1983) with the strong emphasis on the functionality of language and interaction has forced the therapist to move out of the therapy room and into the classroom or every day environment, as the following quotation illustrates: "we cannot expect to be employed by a school if we only provide limited services within a medical model." (simon, 1987:41.) apart from a n e w theoretical or philosophical orientation, focus on the functionality of language brought with it many more issues that confront the language pathologist. for example, if the aim of language intervention is to improve general communicative competence by alleviating language pathology, what exactly is meant by communicative competence and language pathology? firstly, the therapist has to consider the dynamic nature of the concept of'communicative competence'. a functional orientation towards therapy implies that the therapist has to consider the social context within which individuals interact. she has to be aware of the use of language in different contexts in order to plan relevant intervention goals. being aware of the norms of the broader society (or ruling class) as well as the immediate environment of the individual therefore forms the basis of intervention. the therapist can work in a variety of contexts (daily living, the home and work environment, or the academic context), which pressupposes a thorough understanding of communication skills required within these contexts. effective intervention thus necessitates a sound understanding of the interrelationship between the individual and the society, and the specific contexts within which he functions. only by acknowledging the dynamic nature of society can the concept of communication competence be operationalized. secondly, much emphasis has been placed on sensitising therapists to the difference between language pathology (deviance) and language difference within varied linguistic contexts in order to prevent misdiagnosis of language pathology. once again, this implies an active understanding of the linguistic contexts within which nonstandard language variations are used, the cultural implications as well as the problems involved in communicating within the broader societal context. therapists have to be sensitized to work with both bidialectical and bilingual language situations in an attempt not only to diagnose, but also to plan effective and relevant intervention. although basic to the work of the language pathologist, the issue of what constitutes communicative competence and pathology remains complex and difficult to deal with, especially within the south african context. this article aims to describe some of the broader communication needs within the educational system in south africa in order to facilitate the restrucdie suid-afikaanse tydskrif vir kmmunikasiafykins, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 erna alant turing and reformulation of the role of the language therapist within this context. one of the major difficulties confronting any profession is the need for continuous adaptation of training in order to ensure the creation of relevant skills in the context of a changing society (adler, 1988; uys, 1985). it is therefore difficult to describe the role of the speech and language pathologist without some reference to the context within which these professionals have to function. one of the more controversial issues in the american literature in the last decade, has been the involvement of the speech and language therapist in dealing with children who speak nonstandard languages. this problem did not only lead to much critical evaluation and discussion on the effectiveness of evaluation and therapeutic procedures (taylor, 1986; vaughncooke, 1986), but also highlighted the language needs of these children. in this regard, asha (1983) stated that "speechlanguage pathologists may also be available to provide elective clinical service to nonstandard language speakers who do not present a disorder." sol adler (1985), however, describes the approach taken by asha as a "do nothing" or laissez-faire strategy that is totally unacceptable as he believes that the therapist should be involved in the facilitation of bidialectism within the school context. the urgency of this appeal stems from the many research studies (edwards, 1979; taylor, 1986) which confirm the disadvantaged position of these children when entering the school context. this disadvantage also extends to the employment situation when the children are not able to familiarize themselves adequately with the standard language used within a particular society (edwards, 1979; terrell et al. 1983). an equally important assertion by adler (1985) in answer to the position paper of asha (1983) is that the focus on the linguistic aspects of bidialectism and bilingualism detracts from the importance of a sound cultural understanding when working within any communication context. many authors stress the interrelationship between communicative competence in language and understanding of the social context within which that language functions, which supports the issues raised by adler in this regard (e.g. chick, 1987; trudgill, 1987). to become a bidialectical or bilingual speaker involves much more than learning the rules of a particular language. various authors (taylor, 1986; westby and rouse, 1985) in the field of bilingual education have stressed the importance of cultural education in an attempt to enhance bilingualism. for example, the importance of the cultural context in evaluation and remediation is highlighted by taylor (1986:1 1) in the following statement "... the study of normal and pathological communication must be couched ifi cultural terms. to do otherwise is to run the risk of making claims and judgements about the communicative behaviours of a given group of speakers from an inappropriate or, even worse, an ethnocentric set of assumptions and norms." t h e role of the school therapist within an american context it may be useful in attempting to find our bearings in the south african situation to examine the role of the school therapist in the united states. sensitivity to the social context within which the child functions demands that the role of the therapist has to be dynamic in order to comply with the orientations and pressures experienced within a particular school context (marvin, 1987). simon (1987:41) elaborates on this point by stating that "... most change is the result of economic factors. when school districts find they are using 26% of the budget to service 10% of the school population, current practices are scrutinized." it is therefore important that the therapist critically evaluates her own role in the school context and improves her accountability by identifying the most pressing needs of the children in the school and developing the most effective methods for reaching the largest number of children. recent awareness of the therapist's role in nonstandard language as well as the learning disabled contexts stems from a growing realisation of the important role of language within the learning process. the realisation that many students are not successful learners and that there are obvious differences in how children from different social, ethnic and linguistic backgrounds experience the educational set-up contributed to the "rediscovery" of the role language proficiency plays in the educational process (damico, 1987:17). at the same time, the unease many educators displayed when the topic of languageacross-the curriculum was discussed highlighted the fact that "few teachers have the academic background, practical experience and professional freedom to address language-related issues by themselves. consequently language issues are overlooked and children with language differences suffer a variety of abuses within the educational system." (damico, 1987:17.) according to damico (1987) the most likely professional to confront the concerns, abuses and other language related issues in the school is the speech and language pathologist. he emphasizes that the role of the language therapist should be that of a "language specialist" and she should have the flexibility to interact with students and teachers without being tied to a caseload or schedule. this role would involve that the therapist be available to the rest of the school as a consultant on problems relating to language and its impact on classroom activities, testing procedures, culturally diverse populations and other issues. this consultancy would not only involve discussing individual children with the class teacher, but helping to facilitate the student's mastering of the demands made by the school, particularly with regard to the medium of instruct tion or standard language. "no matter what activities are targetted, language is the delivery medium of instruction. u n | less students understand the language, they will not benefit from the instruction." (comkowycz et al. 1987.) ' j ι to facilitate the interaction of the child in the classroom there-1 fore necessitates a closer look at the cultural context and' mother-tongue of the children involved, to ensure that intervention is relevant. it is against this background that the educational context within which the speech and language therapist has to work within south africa will be discussed. the emphasis in the article is on mainstream education although the same issues apply to the special education context. ' t h e educational context in south africa in order to provide some basis from which the speech and language therapist's role in the educational process in south africa can be evaluated,a description of the school context is the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language intervention at schools: changing orientations w i t h i n the s.a. context 11 essential. this will be done by looking at three different aspects: a description of all the children at school in south africa during 1988 according to their race in order to get an overall view of the school population. a description of the different languages used by staff and students in the black schools in 1987. a description of the language used most frequently as the medium of instruction in black schools in 1987. figure 1: pupils to population group for 1 9 8 8 including self-governing states *data obtained from dept of national education (nated 02-214) table 1: mother-tongue language used by students and staff in primary schools in det for 1 9 8 7 * language ! students staff zulu 1 395 090 27 675 / n sotho 649 263 16 633 s sotho 386 547 9 503 xhosa 365 644 9 459 tsongo 286 604 6 485 tswana 215 869 6 576 swazi 183 992 4 444 s ndebele ι 102 683 374 ν ndebele | 33 531 2 091 venda \ 14 316 328 english/afrikaans & other · 124 * data obtained from det annual report (1987) from figure 1 it is clear that the majority of the school-going population in south africa (70,00%) is black, with white children accounting for only 14,41% of the overall population. this paper will therefore concentrate only on this majority group in order to highlight specific issues. one of the major issues in the black school context relates to the different languages used as mother-tongues within this population. table 1 gives some indication of the number of students speaking a particular mother-tongue. it is clear from this table that the diversity of languages poses a problem in terms of which language should be used as the medium of instruction within black schools. the major arguments for and against mother-tongue education will not be discussed here, but the reader can consult reagari (1986) for a summary of the debate. for the purpose of this article it will suffice to quote from the department of education and training's annual report for 1987 in relation to their policy as regards the medium of instruction (det annual report 1987:51.) "from substandard a up to the end of standard 2, the medium of instruction is the pupil's mother-tongue. in substandard β the teaching of one of the official languages is introduced: that is afrikaans or english, according to the parent's choice. the teaching of whichever official language was not chosen in substandard β commences in standard 1. as from standard 3 the medium of instruction is the mother-tongue or one of the two official languages, as decided by the governing body of the school. special attention is given to the official language which serves as medium of instruction. increasingly, the teaching of this language will "cut across the complete curriculum", so that the language becomes an effective medium of instruction. irrespective of the language being used as the medium of instruction, the mother-tongue and the second official language are offered as subjects." from this passage, it becomes clear that firstly the second language that will become the medium for instruction of the school is introduced after one year of schooling (sub b) and is used across the curriculum after three years of exposure to this language. secondly, the mother-tongue is continued in the form of a subject in the school curriculum. apart from an acknowledgement and acceptance of the child's home language, this could reflect a sensitivity in the system towards the "interdependency principle" (kessler, 1984; mcdonald, 1988) which stresses the importance of the development of the first language for the acquisition of the second language. from table 1 it is also clear that a very small percentage of teachers in the det are mother-tongue speakers of english with the majority speaking zulu as their home language. figure 2 provides a clear picture of the languages used as medium of instruction in primary schools. figure 2: enrolment according to medium of instruction in primary schools in det for 1 9 8 7 * * data obtained from det annual report (1987) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasicafivykinys, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 erna alant it is clear that english is the language most commonly used as the medium of instruction, followed by relatively small percentages of a range of other languages. in addition all secondary schools (with only few exceptions) also use english as the medium of instruction. in this regard, johanson (1988) states from observation of students at the university of bophuthatswana that english was widely used, and that only 27% of student communication recorded took place in the mother-tongue. the mother-tongue was mainly used in communication with parents and elderly people, and in community gatherings, churches and courts. in its pure form, the mother-tongue is very seldom used. instead mainly a mixture of languages is used in daily interaction. she concludes that english plays an important, but essentially academic role, and is a lingua franca among the educated and sophisticated. "its importance cannot be questioned, but it is by no means a language of the masses:" (johanson, 1988:2.) obviously, any generalization is problematic, but it seems reasonable to conclude that the urban areas would be less mother-tongue orientated than the rural areas. the implication of these observations is that one can assume a great difference in linguistic background between home and school environments, a factor which contributes to a restricted use of english outside of the academic context. the medium of instruction therefore is (for most of the children) learned and acquired within the school environment, with very little support for the use of english in the actual community (alant, 1988; mawasha 1986). t h e role of t h e speech and language therapist in south african schools a number of important points flow from the above analysis: at least 70% of the total school population (excluding coloured, asian and white children speaking a nonstandard language) come from cultural and linguistic contexts which are different from the standard language or prevailing standard english used in south africa. due to various considerations (reagan, 1986) it is important for these students to obtain entrance into the standard english or ruling-class cultural context to facilitate the acquisition of communicative competence within the content. teachers at black schools are mostly teaching in their second language and have limited communicative competency in standard english which not only inhibits and limits teaching, but also causes a high level of frustration. various authors have also suggested a strong relationship between the above issues and the generally poor performance of students at school (odendaal, 1985; hsrc report 1986). when looking at the overall educational set-up in south africa it is clear that therapists should be involved within this context of second language teaching particularly as regards the prevention of learning problems at schools. the mere fact that far more than half of the children in schools have to study through a second or third language with only limited support from their own environments accentuates the problems that arise from the language issue within the school context (lewis, 1988). under the circumstances, what should the involvement of the language therapist be? traditionally the therapist's primary involvement is with the identification and remediation of communication pathology particularly as this pertains to mother-tongue interaction problems. the underlying philosophy of intervention centers around the individual's basic ability to make contact with people in his immediate environment. obviously the importance of mother-tongue proficiency cannot be underestimated, particularly also in view of its importance for second dialect or second language learning. at the same time, the emphasis on functional communication and the acknowledgement that different situations or contexts require different communication skills necessitates that the therapist has a better understanding of the communication demands in specific contexts. the issue centers around the relevance of facilitating communication skills (e.g. mothertongue) in limited contexts only, thus ignoring interactional difficulties and learning problems the individual might encounter in the broader community. this involvement does not, however, imply that the therapist should become a language teacher. various writers have expressed the opinion that the therapist is not a teacher and that she can therefore not be held responsible for the actual teaching of a second language (taylor, 1986; yoder 1970). as therapists have no background in teaching, this argument is valid. of critical importance to the therapist, however, is the student's competency in the language used as medium of instruction in schools, as this is a prime factor in the development of learning problems. in order to prevent as well as remediate language learning problems at school, the therapist has to be involved in the design and planning of language programmes within the school context. to expect the teacher to take full responsibility for the planning of language programmes and the execution of such programmes seems unrealistic, not only because of the complexity of the phenomenon of second language learning, but also because of the teachers' own lack of proficiency in english, the medium of instruction in the majority of schools (mawasha, 1986). damico's assertion that the language pathologist seems to be the most appropriate person to confront the concerns, abuses and other language related issues in the school seems relevant in that the language pathologist has the linguistic background and the sensitivity towards communication contexts to support arid advise the teacher on strategies and methods that might facilitate language performance (damico, 1987.) j the therapist's role as languag^ consultant within the school context should therefore be emphasized. she has to aid in the planning and execution of in-service training programmes for teachers and although not responsible for the teaching of a second language, should participate extensively in helping teachers to facilitate interaction in the classroom by demonstrating certain communication skills and strategies. the therapist should therefore be available to teachers as a language specialist as damico (1987) suggests. the implication of the above for the training of speech and language therapists within south africa is self-explanatory. firstly, students need a firm grounding not only in the development of the mother-tongues, but also in the development of second language skills. obviously, the emphasis oh the development and remediation of the mother-tongue is of prime importance in providing an adequate basis for general interaction skills. the fact that over 70 per cent of children in south the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language intervention at schools: changing orientations w i t h i n the s.a. context 13 africa s t u d y by m e a n s of a s e c o n d l a n g u a g e n e g a t e s t h e possibility t h a t t h e r a p i s t s c a n r e g a r d t r a i n i n g in s e c o n d l a n g u a g e a c q u i s i t i o n a n d skills as a n 'elective' ( a s h a 1983). t h e t r a i n i n g of s p e e c h a n d l a n g u a g e t h e r a p i s t s i n s o u t h africa h a s b e e n closely l i n k e d t o t h e a m e r i c a n m o d e l . h o w e v e r , in t h e l a s t decade, p a r t i c u l a r l y w i t h t h e s t r e s s o n c o m m u n i t y w o r k ( a r o n , 1 9 8 7 ) a n d c o n s u l t a t i o n (uys, 1 9 8 5 ) t h e r e h a v e b e e n definite a t t e m p t s at c o n t e x t u a l i z i n g t h e profession in o r d e r t o i n c r e a s e its r e l e v a n c e t o d e v e l o p i n g c o u n t r i e s . t h e s e a t t e m p t s c a n o n l y be w e l c o m e d , as t h e y reflect a d y n a m i c m o v e m e n t w h i c h is vital in t h e g r o w t h p r o c e s s of a n y profession. o n l y by o n g o i n g self-evaluation a n d d i s c u s s i o n c a n t h e " e l i t i s m " w h i c h often c h a r a c t e r i z e s p r o f e s s i o n s in d e v e l o p i n g c o u n t r i e s be r e d u c e d i n o r d e r to b e c o m e m o r e r e l e v a n t to t h e p e o p l e m o s t in n e e d of i n t e r v e n t i o n . r e f e r e n c e s adler, s. comment on social dialects. asha, 27, 4, 46-47, 1985. adler, s. a new job description and a new task for the public school clinician. lshss, 19, 1, 28-33, 1988. alant, e. the strengths and weaknesses of written english of black high school pupils. per linguam, 4, 1, 16-23, 1988. aron, m.l. community-based rehabilitation for communication disorders. in w. smith and s. meyer (eds.) sasha national conference proceedings,"wits: johannesburg, 1987. asha social dialects: a position paper. asha, 25, 9, 23-24, 1983. chick, k. interactional perspectives on communication needs of zulu work seekers .journal of multilingual and multicultu ral development, 7, 6, 479-492, 1987. comkowycz, s.m. ehren, b.j. & hayes, ν.,η. meeting classroom needs of language disordered students in middle and junior high school: a program model.journal of childhood communication disorders, 11, 1, 119-208, 1987. damico, j.s. addressing language concerns in the schools: the slp as consultant. journal of childhood commu nication disorders, 11,1, 17-40, 1987. department of education and training (det): det annual report for 1987, rp 65/1988, pretoria: government printer, 1988. department of national education: preliminary education statistics for 1988. nated 02-214 (88/07), pretoria. edwards, j.r. language and disadvantage. london: edward arnold, 1979. gallagher, t.m. and prutting, c.a. pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in langu age.cayifornia: college-hill press, 1983. hsrc-report. the role of language in black education. pretoria: hsrc, 1986. johanson, l. in search for the common tonguenecessity or dream? mathlasedi, 7, 1/2, 7-8, 1988. kessler, c. language acquisition in bilingual children. in n. miller (ed.) bilinguatism and language disability. san diego: collegehill, 1984. lewis, r. memorandum on language-learning deficit in katlehong township. communiphon, sept/oct, 285, 20-21, 1988. marvin, c. a. consultation services: changing roles for the slp's. journal of childhood communication disorders, 11, 1, 1-15, 1987 mawasha, a. l. medium of instruction in black education in southern africa. hsrc-report: the role of language in black education. pretoria: hsrc, 1986. mcdonald, c.a. primary english school curriculumon the threshold of radical change. mathlasedi, 7, 1/2, 18-19,1988. odendaal, m.s. needs analysis of higher primary teachers in kwazulu. per linguam, special issue, 1985. reagan, t. the role of language policy in south african education. language problems and language planning, 10, 1, 1-13, 1986. simon, c.s. out of the broom closet and into the classroom: the emerging slp .journal of childhood communication disorders, 11, 1, 41-66, 1987. taylor, o. treatment of communication disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse populations. san diego: college hill press, 1986. terrell, s.l. and terrell, f. effects of speaking black english upon employment opportunities. asha, 25, 6, 27, 1983. trudgill, ρ accent, dialect and the school. london: edward arnold, 1987. vaughn-cooke, f.b. the challenge of assessing the language of nonmainstream speakers. in o. taylor (ed.) treatment of communication disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse populations. san diego: college hill, 1986. uys, i.c. 'n mede-konsultasie model vir spraaken gehoorterapie in die rsa. in i.s. hay en i.c. uys (eds.) taalverskeidenheid en taalpatologie. pretoria: universiteit van pretoria, 1985. westby, c.e. and rouse, r. culture in education and the instruction of language learning-disabled students. topics in language disorders, 5, 4, 15-28, 1985. yoder, d. some viewpoints of the speech, hearing and language clinician. in f. williams (ed.) language and poverty. chicago: markham, 1970. i 1 v x e d i f i x h e a r i n g s y s t e m c o . cape p . o . b o x 5 2 l y n e d o c h natal p.o. box 4 7 4 4 3 g r e y v i l l e 7 6 0 3 4 0 2 3 p h o n e : ( 0 2 4 ) 5 1 2 4 9 5 p h o n e : ( 0 3 1 ) 2 9 4 0 5 0 surgical & medical p.o. box 19 bedfordview 2008 south africa phone: (011) 53-4188/9 1 r i c h a r d s 2008 south africa phone: (011) 53-4188/9 ir i hearing systems j tomorrow s technology tor today's hearing problem die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasiaykins, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h i s p a g e i s s p o n s o r e d b y t h e l i t e r a r y g r o u p ( p t y ) l t d academic & medical booksellers johannesburg: campus bookshop phone: 34 bertha street (011) 339-1711 2017 braamfontein westdene services 4 ameshoff street 2017 braamfontein phone: (011) 339-3026 durban: logans westdene 660 umbilo road 1400 durban phone: (031) 253221 ext 22 gape town: westdene rondebosch phone: 18 main road (021) 689-4112 7700 rondebosch r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'ν o n d e r s o e k ν α t a a l p r o b l e m e by l e e r g e s t r e m d e kinders. elize d e koker, m . ( l o g . ) ( p r e t . ) department speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg opsomm1ng hierdie studie ondersoek met behulp van die toets vir mondelinge taalproduksie (tmt) (vorster 1980), die taalvermoens van 'n groep van 30 leergestremde kinders. resultate het getoon dat, wat betref mondelinge taalproduksie, die eksperimentele leergestremde groep betekenisvol swakker as die normgroep presteer. die voorkoms en aard van betrokke taalprobleme, asook die nut van die tmt vir gebruik by leergestremde kinders, word bespreek. summary the. study examined the language abilities of a group of 30 learning-disabled children using the "toets vir mondelingse taalproduksie" (tmt) (vorster, 1980). results revealed a significant difference between the scores of the experimental group and the norms of the test. the evidence and nature of proven oral language difficulties were discussed as well as the use of the tmt with learning disabled children. die wetenskaplike vakgebied van "leergestremdheid" het met die verskyning van die murray-verslag in 1969 sy amptelike beslag in suid-afrika gekry. hierdie vakgebied het sy ontstaan oor 'n lang tydperk gehad vanaf 1800 tot 1940 (myers en hammill 1 3 ). hierdie periode word veral gekenmerk deur min of geen empiriese navorsing waar observasies van breinbeseerdes die grondslag van die teoretiese kennis gevorm het. met die verloop van tyd het navorsers daarin geslaag om tussen verskillende soorte leerprobleme te onderskei. u y s 1 7 byvoorbeeld, noem dat verstandelike vertraging, emosionele versteuring en sintuiglike gebreke reeds in die middel van die twintigste eeu as oorsake van leerprobleme bekend was. daar het geleidelik egter 'n nuwe soort leergestremde kind onder die aandag van navorsers gekom, naamlik 'n kind wat nie: verstandelik vertraag sintuiglik gestremd primer emosioneel versteurd of motories gestremd was nie, maar wat wel skolastiese probleme ondervind het en byvoorbeeld nie leer lees, skryf, praat en reken het nie (johnson en myklebust,9). soos byna sonder uitsondering die geval in die "gedragswetenskappe" is, het daar letterlik tientalle definisies, diagnostiese en remedieringsmetodes en benaderings tot hierdie kinders ontstaan. b a r e n 2 noem dat die verskeidenheid van terme wat vir die probleem ontstaan het tot the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) taalprobleme by leergestremde kinders 17 geweldige verwarring aanleiding gegee het. dit is egter nie slegs die terminologie wat kontroversieel geword het nie, maar die hele studiegebied van leergestremdheid. die redes vir die verwarring en kontroversie wat daar heers, le hoofsaaklik in die volgende faktore opgesluit: die leergestremde kind het eerstens 'n baie gevarieerde en wye simptoombeeld. kirk en k i r k 1 0 noem byvoorbeeld dat gestremde kinders nie altyd in sulke goed gedefinieerde kategoriee met homogene eienskappe tuishoort nie._navorsers het dus probleme ondervind om die studiegebied logies en goedgedefinieerd af te baken. tweedens is die leemtes in die kennis ten opsigte van die sentrale senuweestelsel en sy funksionering 'n baie beperkende faktcfr in die studiegebied. derdens, en seker die belangrikste faktor wat ongetwyfeld tot die verwarring moes bydra, was daardie baie denkrigtinge en dissiplines wat hulle met die kinders bemoei, bydraes gelewer, gediagnoseer en behandel het. die spektrum van gei'nteresseerde persone sluit onder andere die volgende in: sielkundiges, onderwysers, sosiale werkers, medici, spraakterapeute, opvoedkundiges, arbeidsterapeute, neuroloe, pediaters, — mense met verskillende teoretiese agtergronde en uitgangspunte. die genoemde verwarring wat daar heers, is ongetwyfeld slegs tot nadeel van die leergestremde kind en daarom moet die oplossing van die verwarring en kontroversie 'n primere doelstelling word. die doelstelling kan onder andere verwesenlik word deur meer en verbeterde empiriese navorsing. daar moet dus gepoog word om die leemtes in die huidige korpus van kennis en navorsing in die studiegebied te identifiseer en nader te bestudeer. probleemstelling slegs een leemte sal vervolgens genoem en bestudeer word naamlik die feit dat die leergestremde kind se taalproblematiek, en dan veral gesproke taal, grootliks in die diagnose en remediering van die kinders oor die hoof gesien is. (wiig en s e m e l , 2 3 hallahan en kauffman,7) 'n bewys vir hierdie stelling is die feit dat hallahan7 drie keer meer artikels in die literatuur vir leergestremdheid ten opsigte van perseptueel — motoriese probleme gevind het, in vergelyking met artikels oor die kinders se moontlike taalprobleme. die perseptueel-motoriese benadering wat tot leerprobleme gevolg word, kan ongetwyfeld weens die pionierswerk van mense soos strauss en cruickshank verantwoord word (myers en hammill, 1 3 ) wallach en goldsmith 2 0 is egter verder van mening dat hierdie benadering nie slegs weens die bydraes van die pioniers op die gebied gevolg word nie, maar ook omdat professionele persone reeds jare die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 elize de koker lank van mening is dat die kern van die leergestremde kind se probleem die perseptueel-motoriese aantasting is. 'n rede hiervoor kan wees dat lees-problematiek met 'n baie hoe frekwensie onder leergestremde kinders voorkom en lees as 'n uitsluitlike visuele taak gesien word. alhoewel dit nie weg te redeneer is dat perseptuele defekte in samehang met leergestremdhede voorkom nie, is hierdie benadering nie 'n bevredigende oplossing nie. baren 2 se byvoorbeeld in die verband dat daar geen bewyse is dat perseptuele vermoens so belangrik vir die leerproses is as wat veronderstel word nie. wallach en goldsmith 2 0 haal "ook ter ondersteuning van bogenoemde stelling vyf outeurs aan wat die volgende mening toegedaan is: . . . question the validity of available visual perceptual testing materials and most importantly question the educational usefulness of the concept of perceptual deficit in treating learning disabilities kritiek kan ook uitgespreek word teen die engheid van 'n perseptuele benadering. daar moet onthou word dat daar verskillende vlakke in die leerproses bestaan. dit is verkeerd om slegs op die hierargies laer vlakke van die leerproses nl. motoriek en persepsie te konsentreer en prosesse soos simbolisering en konseptualisering op die hoere vlakke van die leerproses (myers en hammill, 1 3 johnson en myklebust,9) oor die hoof te sien. ter motivering van 'n taalbenadering tot leergestremde kinders kan daar geen eenvoudiger motivering wees as die feit dat taalprobleme wel 'n deel van die kinders se simptoombeeld vorm nie. daar word spesifiek van 'n leergestremde kind gepraat as gevolg van die opvoedkundige of skolastiese probleme wat hulle ondervind — skolastiese take wat vermoens soos lees, spel, skryf en reken insluit. die genoemde vermoens is almal sekondere taalvermoens. alhoewel monroe (wiig en s e m e l , 2 3 ) reeds in 1932 gevind het dat taalen leerprobleme dieselfde etiologiese faktore toon, kan daar selfs nog aan die begin van die tagtigerjare gese word dat navorsing na die aard en voorkoms van die taalprobleme nog byna afwesig is. die spraakterapeut, onderwyser of opvoedkundige staan dus in die praktyk dikwels magteloos in hulle hantering van taalprobleme wat opgemerk word, veral omdat daar nie diagnostiese toetse bestaan nie en omdat daar oor die algemeen so min kennis en gevolglik min literatuur in die verband beskikbaar is. / dit is dus verstaanbaar dat sommige outoriteite wat by leergestremde kinders betrokke is, orale taalprobleme oor die hoof sal sien en op perseptueel-motoriese probleme sal konsentreer. deur middel van hierdie projek is daar dus gepoog om die genoemde leemte te vul wat veral by die afrikaanse leergestremde kind soveel groter is. daar word ook gehoop dat dit verdere navorsing sal stimuleer en die gebrekkige belangstelling waarvan wiig en s e m e l 2 3 ) praat, sal ophef. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) taalprobleme by leergestremde kinders 19 e k s p e r i m e n t e l e o n t w e r p spesifieke doelstellings en hipotese bewys van orale taalproblematiek deur middel van die eksperimentele metode wat vervolgens bespreek sal word, is daar gepoog om te bewys dat leergestremde kinders taalprobleme het. daar word spesifiek na orale taal gekyk omdat dit die mees waarneembare asook die basiese medium is. die motivering hiervoor is as volg: moran en b y r n e 1 1 noem dat daar tans steeds beperkte bewyse is dat die leergestremde kind se taal van die van die normale kind verskil. vogel1 8 noem die "moontlikheid" dat dislektiese kinders afwykings in orale sintaksis het. uit bogenoemde drie outeurs se bydraes is dit duidelik dat daar nog min wetenskaplike bewyse bestaan, alhoewel outoriteite* spekuleer dat daar wel taalprobleme voorkom. bogenoemde beperkte bewyse en min navorsing word ook deur outeurs soos wiig en semel 2 2 ' 2 3 wiig en r o a c h 2 1 en hallahan7 onderskryf. hallahan en kauffman7 is van mening dat die studies so beperk in getal is en dat daar soveel teenstrydighede bestaan, dat verdere navorsing noodsaaklik is. daar word spesifiek die hipotese gestel dat leergestremde kinders wel gesproke taalprobleme het. voorkoms van taalproblematiek daar is ook gepoog om die vookoms van taalprobleme onder die leergestremde bevolking te bepaal. die motivering vir bogenoemde is as volg: as daar bewys kon word dat leergestremde kinders wel taalprobleme het en dat daar 'n relatiewe hoe insidensie is, gee dit soveel meer rede vir 'n betoog dat die kinders se taal meer aandag moet geniet. daar is spesifiek ouer kinders, m.a.w. sewetot tienjariges in die studie ingesluit. hierdeur is daar gepoog om uitsluitsel te verkry oor die kontroversie wat daar bestaan met betrekking tot die aanwesigheid, al dan nie van simptome van leergestremdheid by ouer kinders. die aard van die taalproblematiek daar is derdens deur hierdie eksperiment gepoog om die aard van die taalprobleme te bestudeer. die volgende vrae kan byvoorbeeld gevra word: — verskil die leergestremde kind se orale taal kwalitatief of kwantitatief van die normale kind s'n? — is daar 'n aparte diagnostiese kategorie waarin die leergestremde bevolking ten opsigte van taalvermoens pas? — wat is die verhouding of onderlinge verband tussen die taal en leerprobleme? die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 elize de koker evaluering van die toets vir mondelinge taalproduksie (tmt) aangesien die toets tans die enigste normatiewe data ten opsigte van sekere aspekte van die ouer afrikaanse kind se taalvermoe verskaf is dit belangrik dat die waarde van die toets se gebruik by leergestremde kinders bepaal sou word. seleksie van proefpersone kriteria vir seleksie in 'n poging om die orale taal van leergestremde kinders met die van normales te vergelyk, is daar van 'n eksperimentele, sowel as kontrole groep (eg en kg) gebruik gemaak. kontrole groep die kg is die verteenwoordigende steekproef waarop die norms van die tmt gestandardiseer is. eksperimentele groep die volgende kriteria is aan die groep gestel: (i) kinders met 'n leergestremdheid moes geselekteer word. omdat daar soveel moontlike interpretasies van die term bestaan, word die volgende definisie as kriterium gebruik: die kinders mag nie — opvallende fisiese of sintuiglike gebreke gehad het nie — ernstig emosioneel versteur gewees het nie — gemanifesteerde epilepsie of serebrale verlamming ondervind het nie — daar moes wel sagte neurologiese tekens teenwoordig gewees het (ii) omdat 'n ondergemiddelde intelligensie taaltoetse nadelig bei'nvloed, is daar vereis dat proefpersone (pp) 'n gemiddelde intelligensie sou he (m.a.w. 100+) (iii) skoolgaande kinders is geselekteer omdat intelligensie .syfers by hulle meer betroubaar is. leergestremdheid is by hulle ook reeds gei'dentifiseer en daar kon so ook afleidings gemaak word t.o.v. die aanwesigheid van taalproblematiek tydens stadia waar dit nie meer verwag word nie (schain1 6) .(iv) dertig proefpersone is geselekteer om statisties betekenisvolle resultate te kon verkry 7 (v) emosionele onstabiliteit, sosio-'ekonomiese status, sowel as geslag is as moontlike bei'nvloedende faktore in ag geneem. eersgenoemde twee faktore is deur ewekansige steekproeftrekking geassimileer. omdat leergestremdheid baie meer by seuns as dogters aanwesig is, is daar besluit op 'n ewekansige steekproef. daar sal dus noodwendig meer' seuns as dogters in die steekproef wees wat 'n getroue weergawe van die verskynsel is. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) taalprobleme by leergestremde kinders 21 verkryging van die proefpersone die kinders is uit hulpklasse van die t . o . d . verkry. die proefpersone is spesifiek uit die totale bevolking van hulpklaskinders in pretoria gekies omdat die brandstofkrisis 'n groter area onmoontlik gemaak het. daar bestaan in pretoria vyftien hulpklasse, elk met 8 leerlinge, met ander woorde, 'n totale bevolking van ± 120 leerlinge. 'n verteenwoordigende en ewekansige steekproef van 30 leerlinge tussen die ouderdomme 7-4 tot 9-10 is uit bg. bevolking getrek. eksperimentele materiaal die tmt is gebruik asook — 'n bandmasjien (draagbare sony) — lae-geruis/kasette (hitachi) — die ,,samevattende verslag oor 'n leerling met leergestremdhede" (t.o.d. omsendminuut 73 van 1979 — bylaag a ) 1 5 die verslag is deur die betrokke ortodidaktikus wat die hulpklaskinders geselekteer het, voltooi. daaruit is agtergrond-historiese gegewens vir elke pp ingewin. prosedure die toets is volgens spesifikasies, bv. met betrekking tot omgewing, aanwysings en prosedure afgele. statist1ese verwerking die proefpersone se roupunt vir elkeen van die 16 sub-toetse van die toets is met behulp van die normtabelle na persentielrange omgesit. laasgenoemde kon egter nie vir statistiese verwerking gebruik word nie en is daarom na staneges verwerk. ten einde te bepaal of die pp se prestasie as groep betekenisvol van die normgroep verskil het, is die t-toets vir statisties betekenisvolle verskille op die 5% en 2\% vlak van betekenisvolheid uitgevoer. resultate. (sien tabelle i i i i ) die resultate word kortliks aan die hand van die spesifieke doelstellings bespreek. vir 'n meer gedetaileerde bespreking word u verwys na de koker. 5 bewys van orale problematiek bewyse het wel deeglik uit die eksperimentele navorsing na vore getree. die eerste bewys is reeds uit die leerlinge se agtergrond-gegewens verkry. daar -is gevind dat 'n minimum van 73,3% van die pp 'n geskiedenis van spraaken taalverwante probleme gehad het. daar was bv. by 40% van die eg spesifiek genoem dat hulle laat begin praat het. hierdie verskynsel is ook deur m u r r a y 1 2 genoem. uit tabel ii — blyk dit dat die eg in die tmt op 12 uit die 16 subtoetse betekenisvol swakker as die normale kind presteer het die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) elize de koker η s η .53 •6 c a > u "m μ (λ c *{λ a c e υ <υ "ο ο. ο c ο ιλ <υ ο. μ ι υ ο c α > <υ μ c α c <υ <υ cl, -ι μ (0 < η | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 ο en no s no ts ο en ο on on ĉ ο no en en •st ts no ĉ •st d oo ο | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 on cn £ no ο ο ο ο no no cn no in m oo m 00 oo no •st ο | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 oo cn ο en tf no no •st iri en no cn no no ο 3 on oo no on •st no | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 cn no ο 00 on •st ts •st ο) cn no ts ο •st •st no no •st ts ts •st 00 no •st | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 no cn no no ο oo ron en on no no t̂ 00 00 00 no on o) | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 cn on ο en ο on •st ο in on ĉ ο no no r-ts oo rno no oo no ο cn •st in | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 •st ts ο no no no •st cn cn oo on no on en no •st •st no on ts •st no no ts 00 no no •st •st | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 ο ts ο ts ο no on ĉ ο cn no rno •st 00 00 00 no en | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 cs cn) oo •st in ο on en no ο rο cn 00 ο cn ĉ r•st -h oo no en in r| 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 ts en •st on no in t— cn on en ο cn no •st m ĉ ĉ oo on t— en ts | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 ο ts oo •st ts in oo •st on 00 ο en on ο •st cn 00 oo no t— 00 no rts | 74 75 76 78 78 79 79 7-1 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 8-0 0 800 8 -1 8 -2 8 -4 8 -4 8 -6 8 -7 8 -8 8 -9 8 -9 8 -10 9 -0 9 -00 9 -0 9 -2 9 -2 9 -3 9 -10 1 on •st (n o) wi 00 on ro) ts on m ο oo r-•st 00 t— 00 r00 no on a (λ "u ιλ ω ιλ ο ο > d. a> •o kh ο ο ι d3 a 1λ 00 c 'w c8 "a <υ > q. ml c3 "ob ω a (λ 0d c 'eb c5 ca & > u ό ο ο-ο ο ϋ οχ. ο c (λ an a •c ω ·£ ή ω ο έ | u w-ιζι a 1λ 00 x; t-l u κ a [λ 00 c ε-υ ο c a> m •a .'δ / ί a> a> ξ j2 » ο ί · 8 ιλ j) ca αϊ a υ c 'ξ ce o ud er do m m e 10 years 2 4% total 50 100% the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the current practices, training and concerns of a group of hospital-based speech therapists working in the area of dysphagia 7 from participation in the field study. based on the results of the pilot study, various changes were made to the questionnaire. field study the heads of the speech therapy departments at the 24 hospitals involved were thus contacted telephonically. information about the purpose of the study was provided and the number of therapists working in each department was confirmed. questionnaires were then posted to the various departments and due dates for their return was stipulated in the cover letters. a follow-up telephone call was then made approximately one week later to ascertain whether the departments had received the questionnaires. data analysis descriptive statistics were employed to analyse the responses obtained from the closed-ended items in the questionnaire, while content analysis was used to analyse open-ended questions qualitatively according to themes expressed by respondents. results and discussion to the years of working experience of these therapists, it emerged that almost one-fifth (18%) of the respondents were working in departments where all the therapists had less than three years' working experience i.e. they were all junior therapists. even more disconcerting was the finding that 8% of these respondents were working alone in their departments with no support from other junior or more senior experienced staff. these results suggested that many of the respondents might have been experiencing difficulty in obtaining the necessary support and guidance from more experienced or senior colleagues. it was therefore recommended that efforts be made to organise such supervision/mentor systems in conjunction with other hospitals and with university staff members. the need for supervision has been highlighted as an ethical responsibility for clinicians with limited experience (aash, 1994: sashla, 1998) a further finding in this section was that respondents received most referrals of dysphagia patients from doctors (82%) and considerably fewer referrals from allied health disciplines. this finding suggested poor knowledge of the role of slps in dysphagia by other health professionals. the need for slps to advocate their role and services in dysphagia as well as to educate health professionals was thus highlighted. self-administered questionnaires were sent out to 75 slps working in the major provincial hospitals around gauteng, kwa-zulu natal and the cape province, from which a 67% response rate was obtained. results are discussed in accordance with the sub-aims of the study. current practices investigation of respondents' current practices in dysphagia revealed that the overwhelming majority of respondents (90%) were involved in assessing and treating dysphagia patients. a minimum of 1-5 patients were seen by most respondents (62%) on a monthly basis. almost half (46%) of the respondents also reported that they were involved in student training and this training included the area of dysphagia management. these findings highlighted ythe need for respondents to be adequately trained in dysphagia practice at an undergraduate level. this need was further emphasised by the fact that many respondents were working in small departments that had a limited number of more experienced senior staff. opportunities for supervision and mentoring were thus thought to be limited. the results indicated that the majority of the respondents (56%) were working in small departments consisting of only 1-4 therapists. when this fact was related the training of respondents in dysphagia undergraduate training results displayed in table 2 indicate that dysphagia was covered in the vast majority (96%) of the respondents' undergraduate courses. amost disconcerting finding related to two respondents who reported that they were actively involved in assessing and treating dysphagia patients despite not having received any basic training in dysphagia at an undergraduate level. although both respondents did report attendance at continuing education courses and workshops, the exact level of this input could not be ascertained. this finding has serious implications as the code of ethics (saslha, 1997) clearly stipulates that slps should provide only those services for which they hold the appropriate qualification and for which they are competent by training. a lack of adherence to this principle is potentially harmful to the client, particularly in the area of dysphagia practice where improper management may have life-threatening consequences (cannito, 1995). within the group of 48 respondents who received dysphagia training in their undergraduate studies, more than two-thirds (73%) reported that dysphagia was not table 2: dysphagia courses in undergraduate training dysphagia included in undergraduate courses n=50 no yes no. 2 48 percent 4% 96% whether dysphagia was included as a separate or integrated course n=48 number percent separate module 13 27% integrated course 35 73% die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 naina modi & eleanor ross taught as a separate module, but rather integrated into other speech pathology courses. toner (1995) suggests that this integration of the dysphagia module is typical in small speech therapy departments where the teaching load already exceeds university recommendations and therefore precludes the addition of new courses. he further states that this situation frequently results in a focus on theory with limited time for observation and hands-on experience, and therefore recommends that a separate course in dysphagia is a necessity. approximately half (48%) of the respondents reported that their undergraduate speech therapy departments had not been linked to medically-based practicum sites. however, students are generally required to complete a clinical block at a hospital setting, thus it can be assumed that dysphagia practice is incorporated into the student training by such hospital-based clinicians. with regard to the theoretical areas in the undergraduate courses as suggested by asha (1989), cst(1990) andaash (1994), most of the respondents were apparently taught about the anatomy and physiology of swallowing (86%), medical conditions and disorders related to swallowing (73%), assessment methods in dysphagia (79%) and management procedures in dysphagia (75%). however, the areas of legal and ethical issues (52%), the roles of team members (50%) and safety and emergency procedures (65%) and did not appear to be covered in the majority of the respondents' undergraduate. these results are summarised in table 3. findings in respect of safety and emergency procedures are particularly disturbing if one considers that hospitalbased slps are frequently faced with complex, medically unstable dysphagia patients. consequences such as choking, aspiration and malnutrition are therefore a daily reality for dysphagia clinicians in acute care settings (mirro and patey, 1991). it is thus essential for clinicians to have adequate knowledge of emergency and safety techniques in order to prevent or intervene in the event of medical complications (asha, 1987; cst, 1990). although clinicians may often not be personally trained in emergency procedures, they still need to ensure that appropriate safety equipment and trained personnel are available during potentially dangerous situations (aash, 1994). clinicians should also be aware of local policy and guidelines in the area of crossinfection (aash, 1994; saslha, 1998). the limited number of respondents who reported receiving this input during their undergraduate training is very disconcerting. saslha (1998) lists routine precautions for infection control in a hospital setting but no specific attention is paid to the area of dysphagia. an added concern is the lack of attention in the document on the precaution against hiv transmission, especially in view of current health policy stressing that health care workers are in high risk situations for hiv transmission (college of medicine of south africa, 1991). it is therefore recommended that saslha use kulpa's (1990) guidelines for slps with regard to safety precautions against hiv transmission and management of patients. practical clinical training at an undergraduate level was investigated according to the guidelines provided by cst (1990), asha (1991) and aash (1994). according to table 4, the vast majority of the respondents reported that they did not receive any level of the recommended practical training at the undergraduate level. this is a disconcerting finding as these guidelines serve to ensure that students receive comprehensive clinical training. while it may be understandable that independent assessment and treatment of dysphagia patients may not be allowed due to the high risk nature of the area, this factor does not account for students not practicing techniques on normal subjects or even observing and assisting qualified professionals. based on these findings, it is not surprising that the overwhelming majority (92%) of the respondents rated their undergraduate training as poor. the most common reason given by respondents was the lack of practical training included in their undergraduate courses. similar findings were obtained in an asha standards validation study which aimed to validate the professional domains, tasks, knowledge and skills in the general practice of speechlanguage pathology (cited in bernthal and bankson, 1992). respondents recommended an increase in practical training hours in order to ensure that students have a variety of experience with individuals with various disorders of differing severity. other reasons provided by respondents for rating their undergraduate training as poor, included the lack of a specific dysphagia module, the limited number of lectures on dysphagia, an insufficient amount of information covered theoretically, poor structuring of the course and a lack of exposure to teamwork. postgraduate training almost two-thirds of the respondents (64%) in the study indicated that they had attended conferences, lectures or areas covered in courses not covered in courses no responses anatomy / physiology 86% 6% 8% medical conditions 73% 21% 6% assessment 79% 13% 8% management 75% 17% 8% ethical issues 35% 52% 13% safety issues 25% 65% 10% roles of team members 50% 40 10% table 3. theoretical areas in dysphagia covered at an undergraduate level (n = 48) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the current practices, training and concerns of a group of hospital-based speech therapists working in the area of dysphagia 9 workshops after graduation. respondents' attendance at postgraduate courses indicates their awareness of their responsibility to continue their education after qualification in order to maintain a high standard of professional competence (saslha, 1997; saslha, 1998). technological advances and everincreasing knowledge bases demand that clinicians acquire current levels of expertise through various forms of continuing education. almost half (47%) of the postgraduate courses attended by respondents were one day in length. more importantly, none of the courses extended for as long as a week and therefore did not comply with recommendations by logemann (1995) in terms of length of course, namely 5-6 workshops, each of 2-3 days in length. such courses are probably inappropriate in the south african situation where staff and financial resources for rehabilitation services are poor (white paper for the transformation of the health system in south africa, 1997). clinicians therefore have to maximise existing resources by considering their access to the major providers of continuing professional education in speech-language pathology. rassi and mcelroy (1992a) identify these providers as being: a) college and universities; b) state and national professional associations and public agencies; c) hospitals and clinics, and d) independent providers such as publishers and equipment manufacturers. the content included in the courses attended by the respondents was investigated according to the theoretical areas recommended by asha(1989), cst (1990) and aash (1994). these results are summarised in table 3.3. the vast majority of respondents (94%) reported the inclusion of anatomy and physiology related to dysphagia. all the respondents (100%) also reported that the procedures and techniques of dysphagia assessment and management were covered in their postgraduate courses. in contrast, only about half of the respondents reported the inclusion of interdisciplinary teaching and clinical practice on normal subjects and dysphagia patients. this finding is disconcerting as a lack of multidisciplinary management in dysphagia has been shown to lead to fragmented patient care (lefton-grief and arvedson, 1997). the limited inclusion of clinical practice with both normal subjects and with dysphagia patients in respondents' postgraduate courses is also of great concern in view of the restricted hands-on training received by most respondents during their undergraduate courses. this finding is not unique to the south african situation. inherent in this finding is the lack of recognition that practical training plays a vital and integral role in the educational preparation of slps (rassi and mcelroy, 1992b). respondents in the study thus apparently had limited opportunities for application of theoretical knowledge in the clinical setting during both undergraduate and postgraduate training. once again, the table 4: practical training in dysphagia at an undergraduate level (n=47)* / / practical areas covered in training not covered in training practising dysphagia assessment and treatment with normal subjects 4% 96% observing dysphagia assessment and treatment with dysphagia patients 34% 66% assisting with dysphagia assessment and treatment of dysphagia patients under supervision 15% 85% ' independent dysphagia assessment and treatment of dysphagia patients 4% 96% interdisciplinary dysphagia assessment and treatment of dysphagia patients 4% 96% * not applicable to 2 respondents; 1 respondent failed to respond table 5. content of postgraduate courses in dysphagia (n=38) areas covered in courses not covered in courses no responses anatomy/physiology 94% 3% 3% assessment procedures & techniques 100% 0% 0% management procedures and techniques 100% 0% 0% interdisciplinary teaching 56% 38% 6% clinical practice on normal subjects 50% 47% 3% clinical practice on dysphagia patients 53% 44% 3% die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 naina modi & eleanor ross findings highlight the need for the professional organisation for slps to develop a position paper on dysphagia so that appropriate standards of postgraduate courses can be established and maintained. only half of the respondents (50%) who attended postgraduate training rated it as good. a further 44% of the respondents rated their training as average, while a limited number of respondents (6%) felt that the training was poor. various positive aspects of the postgraduate courses were described by respondents. the most common aspect related to the opportunity for practical training and the fact that the trainers were adequately qualified experts working in the field. other positive factors included the adequacy of the course content in terms of material not covered at an undergraduate level, information on new advances in the field, input from various professionals and the relevance of material to the hospital environment. a focus on multidisciplinary work and counselling skills as well as specific training on interpreting normal and abnormal videofluroscopic studies was also described as being particularly beneficial. as with the undergraduate training, limitations of postgraduate courses described by respondents related most frequently to the lack of practical training included in the courses. role of the speech therapist with regard to the role of the slp in dysphagia, the vast majority of the respondents showed an awareness that the slp should be involved in all areas including identification of dysphagia patients (90%), assessing and treating dysphagia patients (100%), establishing and co-ordinating a multidisciplinary team (78%), providing information to patients and caregivers (100%),educating other health professionals about dysphagia (100%) and conducting research on normal and abnormal swallowing (98%). this finding was encouraging as these roles of the slp are emphasized in the literature and by the various professional organizations. the overwhelming majority of the respondents (90%) felt that the role of the slp in dysphagia management was only partially recognised by their medical colleagues. the most common reason cited by respondents for the lack of full recognition by medical colleagues related to the lack of knowledge not only of their role in dysphagia but also the general areas of work of a speech therapist. respondents claimed that this lack of knowledge was evident in the limited number of referrals generally and the number of inappropriate referrals received. respondents reported that referrals were made by a restricted number of doctors usually older doctors who had been educated by speech therapists over the years or newly trained doctors who had a rehabilitation week included in their undergraduate training. underlying reasons suggested by respondents for this poor knowledge included the lack of information on rehabilitation provided to doctors at an undergraduate level and the fact that most speech therapy departments were not situated at medical schools where doctors are trained. respondents also felt that the limited numbers of speech therapists in the hospital setting as well as general time constraints severely restricted attendance at ward rounds and participation in teamwork as well as opportunities for initiating contact with doctors. the majority of the respondents (71%) felt that their role in dysphagia was only partially recognised by their paramedical colleagues. as with medical staff, the main reason offered by respondents for the lack of full recognition, related to the limited knowledge about the specific role of the speech therapist in dysphagia. respondents felt that this lack of knowledge was reflected in the small number of paramedical staff who referred patients, the restricted number of referrals received, the fact that most dysphagia patients were initially referred only as speech/language patients, and the fact that only patients with severe dysphagia symptoms were referred. these findings serve to highlight the importance of interdisciplinary teaching at an undergraduate level so that there is carryover into the work setting after qualification. respondents also highlighted the critical need for clinicians to actively market their role in dysphagia practice. knowledge and perceptions of multidisciplinary teamwork in dysphagia all the respondents (100%) felt that teamwork was role of the slp yes no ; identification of dysphagia patients 90% 10% i assessment of dysphagia patients 100% 0% treatment of dysphagia patients 100% 0% establishing a multidisciplinary team 92% 8% co-ordinating a multidisciplinary team 78% 22% providing information to patients and caregivers 100% 0% education of other health professionals 100% 0% conducting research on normal and abnormal swallowing 98%* 0% *1 respondent did not respond table 6: role of the speech-language pathologist in dysphagia (n=50) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the current practices, training and concerns of a group of hospital-based speech therapists working in the area of dysphagia 11 necessary for assessing and managing dysphagia patients. several investigators have in fact reported that comprehensive treatment by an interdisciplinary team has led to improved management of patients' nutritional status, reduced likelihood of complications, greater cost-effectiveness of treatment and earlier discharge of patients (young and durant-jones, 1990). furthermore, the majority of respondents showed an awareness of which professionals comprise a multidisciplinary dysphagia team. the overwhelming majority of the respondents included all the professionals listed as part of the dysphagia team, namely dieticians, physiotherapists, occupational therapists, neurologists, doctors, nurses, radiologists and ear, nose and throat specialists. access to essential resources respondents' access to essential resources for dysphagia practice was investigated. these sources included various diagnostic procedures, suctioning equipment, diet modification services, dysphagia team members and supervision / mentor systems. with regard to diagnostic procedures, the overwhelming majority of the respondents (98%) indicated that they had access to x-rays as a diagnostic procedure. only approximately half of the respondents (54%) indicated that they had access to videofluoroscopy. almost one-third (32%) did not have access, while a further 14% failed to respond to the question. this relatively high rate of a lack of response could be related to respondents being unaware of whether the diagnostic procedure was available or not, which could imply limited use of the procedure in dysphagia management. this finding is disconcerting as videofluoroscopy, specifically the modified barium swallow has been described as the 'gold standard' in the evaluation and treatment of dysphagia (mchorney and rosenbek, 1998). a number of implications arise from the responses furnished by clinicians in the study. firstly, there is a need for respondents to be aware of the specific uses of videofluoroscopy in dysphagia assessment and treatment. efforts also need to be made by respondents to find out about access to this diagnostic procedure. lastly, if there is a lack of access to video/fluoroscopy, respondents need to be aware of alternative diagnostic procedures as well as the need for more conservative management as suggested by groher (1995). comprehensive knowledge of assessment procedures and techniques thus needs to be provided at an undergraduate level. | many of the respondents (32%) failed to respond to the question about access to ultrasound and a further 6% indicated that they were unsure of availability of this procedure in their work settings. hence, it can be postulated from these results that limited use was made of this procedure as a diagnostic tool even if it was available. the results also suggest poor knowledge of instrumental procedures by many respondents. a very limited number of respondents (38%) indicated that they had access to ultrasound. the unique characteristics of ultrasound make the procedure an important part of the comprehensive diagnosis of dysphagia. limited access and possibly poor knowledge of the procedure thus places respondents and patients at a distinct disadvantage for more comprehensive holistic care. increased understanding of instrumentation in dysphagia therefore needs to be facilitated at an undergraduate level. almost half of the respondents (46%) failed to respond to the question about scintigraphy and a further 8% indicated that they were unsure of access to this procedure in their work settings. once again, it can be postulated that this finding reflected limited knowledge of the procedure and limited use of it as a diagnostic tool even if it was available. only 10% of the respondents indicated access to scintigraphy while 36% were sure about not having access to the procedure. this finding is not surprising particularly in the south african situation considering that scintigraphy is both costly and requires specialised nuclear medicine expertise (sonies, 1991). the present focus of the south african health department is to shift expenditures and commission equipment for the delivery of primary health care services (white paper for the transformation of the health system in south africa, 1997). in line with this focus, national policy and guidelines have been developed for essential technology. similar findings in relation to previous diagnostic procedures were obtained with respect to fibreoptic endoscopic examination of swallowing (fees). almost half (46%) of the respondents failed to respond while a further 10% indicated that they were unsure of access to fees. an extremely limited number of respondents (8%) indicated that they had access to fees, while 36% did not have access to this procedure. the accuracy of this finding is questionable if one considers that this procedure is routinely used by ents for assessing the integrity of the laryngeal and pharyngeal structures (logemann, 1998). it is therefore disconcerting that many of the respondents appeared to be unaware of access to this procedure and were therefore presumably making limited use of it as an effective tool for dysphagia management. with regard to electromyography (emg), almost half of the respondents (46%) failed to respond and a further 6% indicated that they were unsure of whether emg was available in their work settings. a mere 10% of the respondents indicated access to emg while more than onethird (38%) were sure about inaccessibility. it is important for clinicians to be aware of the specific use of this diagnostic procedure as an assessment and therapeutic tool in dysphagia, especially in the presence of neuromuscular disorders. the procedure may also prove to be useful in the absence of other procedures, especially in the south african situation where resources are limited. there are therefore serious concerns regarding the large number of respondents who appeared to be unaware of the availability of emg as well as the large number who did not have access to this resource. appropriate referrals for specific patients are therefore compromised and this situation, in turn, may impact on early diagnosis and comprehensive care. the results of access to manometry followed a very similar pattern to those for the other diagnostic procedures. the largest proportion of the respondents (46%) did not respond to the question, while 8% indicated their uncertainty about access to manometry. only 8% of the respondents indicated definite access to the procedure, while 38% clearly had no access to this resource. almost one-third of the respondents (31%) indicated that they did not have access to suctioning equipment in their work settings. this result was surprising as one would have expected the standard availability of suction equipment in a hospital setting where it is regularly used by nurses and physiotherapists in patients with chest conditions. it can therefore be postulated that respondents had limited die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 naina modi & eleanor ross knowledge about suction equipment and were therefore unaware of available access to it. this finding is extremely worrying as suctioning can play a critical role during management of dysphagia patients. limited knowledge of access to suction equipment suggests that respondents were not familiar with safety and emergency procedures in the event of choking or aspiration. this finding is not surprising in view of earlier findings that almost two-thirds (65%) of the respondents reported that safety and emergency procedures were not covered in their undergraduate courses. poor knowledge of these procedures has potentially lifethreatening implications for patients, especially those with chronic pulmonary disease. there is therefore an urgent need for undergraduate curricula in dysphagia to incorporate the area of safety and emergency procedures. furthermore, a dysphagia position paper by saslha could serve to highlight potential risk factors in dysphagia and the necessary precautions that need to be undertaken. the overwhelming majority of respondents (94%) indicated that they had access to diet modification facilities. this finding is important for dysphagia management as diet modification is a specific strategy in the assessment and treatment of swallowing disorders. standard hospital diets often pose problems for dysphagia patients. the overwhelming majority of the respondents indicated that they had access to all the team members mentioned i.e. dietician, physiotherapist, occupational therapist, neurologist, doctor, nurse, radiologist and ent. the results indicate that more than half (54%) of the respondents did not have any system of supervision/ mentoring operating in their departments. cst (1990) and aash (1994) state that it is the medico-legal and ethical responsibility of slps who are aware that they lack the desired level of skill to seek guidance from more senior or experienced staff before intervening in management of the patient. the principle of ethics ii (asha, 1994) clearly stipulates that clinicians should engage in only those aspects of the profession that are within the scope of their competence considering the level of their education, training and experience. saslha (1998) also states that negligence can be proved in instances where it is clear that intervention should have been undertaken by a more experienced clinician or the inexperienced therapist should have requested supervision. the lack of respondents' access to a system of supervision or mentoring in dysphagia management has serious implications. without the institution of an organised system of supervision or mentoring, many of the respondents might not have been getting the necessary support and assistance from more experienced staff and the level of clinical service might not have been optimal for the patient. this possibility, in turn, raises questions about quality assurance at the work settings in the study. just under half of the respondents (46%) indicated that there were systems of supervision or mentoring operating in their departments. there was a combination of both formal and informal systems being implemented at the time of the study, and this situation depended largely on the presence and level of experience of more senior staff. there appeared to be a specific focus in many departments on new graduates. these clinicians were either specifically assigned to senior supervisors or were simply made aware of available assistance whenever needed. some of the respondents also reported that their heads of department and senior staff took responsibility for organising regular case discussions and continuing education and also encouraged new graduates to attend outside courses and conferences. rassi and mcelroy (1992a) found that this responsibility of senior staff for more junior personnel was important as most professionals cannot easily identify their learning needs and therefore require guidance and assistance in structuring their continuing professional education. an overwhelming majority (98%) of the respondents felt that a system of supervision or mentoring for dysphagia treatment was important. they linked the need for supervision to the life-threatening nature of the work and many expressed concern about the safety of the patients. a number of the respondents also remarked on the challenging, stressful and frightening nature of dysphagia management. the lack of confidence and feeling of incompetence in new graduates and junior therapists was a frequently cited concern and these feelings were linked by many respondents to limited undergraduate training in dysphagia, especially with respect to the practical component. it was felt that supervision and mentoring would allow for the development of experience and confidence, as well as for accountability to the patient and the profession as a whole. respondents felt that supervision would give therapists the opportunity to recognise their own limitations and thereby not compromise optimal care of the patient. the large caseload of dysphagia patients within hospitals was also of concern to respondents and the need for quality assurance measures was highlighted. in addition, respondents recognised that dysphagia management is a growing field with new advancements occurring all the time. therefore the need for sharing information, knowledge and skills was vital. respondents' concerns in this area highlight the need for a position paper in south africa which could specify the need for supervision as formulated by cst (1990) and aash (1994). knowledge and competence knowledge almost half of the respondent (46%) rated their knowledge on human anatomy related to dysphagia as being average while a further 42% rated their knowledge as being good. this finding was not surprising as the majority of the respondents indicated that they had covered this aspect theoretically in their undergraduate training. in addition, the theory on anatomy was covered in 94% of the postgraduate courses attended by respondents. j the majority of the respondents (58%) rated their knowledge of the risk factors in dysphagia as being good. this finding was not unexpected as the majority of the respondents (70%) were exposed to the various medical conditions and disorders resulting in dysphagia at an undergraduate level. the majority of the respondents rated their knowledge about instrumental procedures as poor (36%) or average (38%). earlier results had indicated that assessment procedures and techniques had been covered in the majority of respondents' undergraduate and postgraduate courses. however, respondents' knowledge and awareness of access to specific instrumental procedures was found to be limited when access to resources was probed. this finding suggests that respondents' knowledge might have been restricted to commonly used procedures such as x-rays and videofluoroscopy and their limited knowledge about other procedures the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the current practices, training and concerns of a group of hospital-based speech therapists working in the area of dysphagia 13 may have been highlighted by the question on access to resources. the largest proportion of the respondents (48%) rated their knowledge of alternate feeding methods as good. this finding could be anticipated in view of the fact that procedures and techniques of dysphagia management were reported to have been covered theoretically in the majority of the respondents' undergraduate training. the majority of the respondents (65%) rated their knowledge of the roles of key dysphagia team members as good. this finding can only be partially attributed to respondents' training as the roles of team members were reportedly covered in only approximately half of the respondents' undergraduate and postgraduate courses. it can be postulated that respondents' self-perceived 'good' knowledge of the roles was also related to their access and exposure to various team members in their work settings. the majority of the respondents rated their knowledge about medical terminology and conditions associated with dysphagia as being either average (40%) or good (42%), i.e. only 18% rated their knowledge as poor. this finding is not surprising if one considers that this area was covered in the majority (70%) of the respondents' undergraduate training. skills just under half of the respondents (47%) rated their skills in assessing dysphagia patients as average i.e. a limited number of respondents (29%) rated their skills as good. this finding was not surprising considering the fact that the vast majority of respondents reported limited opportunities for practical training in both undergraduate and postgraduate courses attended. these results suggest that the provision of theoretical knowledge without the accompanying practical experience was perceived by respondents as inadequate in terms of preparation of their skills for use in the workplace. this finding of a lack of practical training in dysphagia courses is well documented in the literature (toner, 1995). , inadequate skills with regard to assessing patients has serious implications for the safety of patients as clinicians /need to make accurate diagnoses in order to implement appropriate treatment. the life-threatening risks of choking and aspirating demand ajhigh level of skill in assessment of dysphagia patients and highlights, once again, the need for position statements which guide effective training of students at an undergraduate level and clinicians at a postgraduate level. i as with the assessment of dysphagia patients, the largest proportion of the respondents (49%) rated their skills in treating dysphagia patients as being average. similar reasons can be postulated for this finding i.e. the limited practical hands-on training gained by the respondents during their undergraduate and postgraduate courses. feelings of emotional comfort or confidence the largest proportion of the respondents (41%) rated their feelings of emotional comfort or confidence in handling dysphagia patients as average. inadequate theoretical and practical training provided at courses attended, large case loads, limited knowledge of safety and emergency procedures and limited access to support systems from more experienced professionals were probably all factors contributing to decreased emotional comfort and confidence levels in handling dysphagia patients. a possible implication of such feelings is that clinicians may then not be willing to engage in active advocacy regarding the professionals' role in dysphagia and may also project a poor image of the profession by not being assertive in their knowledge, skills and decision-making. probably one of the best ways of improving respondents' emotional comfort and confidence levels in handling patients is to ensure effective training at an undergraduate level. the majority of the respondents rated their feelings of emotional comfort or confidence in counselling the families of dysphagia patients as being average (37%) or good (39%). a possible explanation for respondents being more comfortable or confident with counselling the families as compared to actual handling of patients is that respondents might have been experienced with counselling in general as it is incorporated regularly into all areas of work in speech and language pathology. furthermore, respondents had generally rated their knowledge about dysphagia better than their skills in handling dysphagia patients. a large component of counselling families includes the sharing of information and therefore requires a good theoretical knowledge base. general views and concerns at the end of the questionnaire, respondents were given the opportunity to express their general views and concerns about the area of dysphagia. the types of concerns cited by respondents included inadequate training in dysphagia at an undergraduate level (particularly limited hands-on practical training and lack of knowledge of safety and emergency procedures); poor knowledge of the role of the slp in dysphagia by other health professionals; limited resources available (including objective diagnostic instrumentation, suction equipment, postgraduate courses and continuing education, experienced contact people in the field, monthly support groups with more experienced clinicians, and guidance and supervision within the work setting); and the need for quality assurance measures. respondents' suggestions for overcoming these concerns related to improving undergraduate and postgraduate training, maximising limited resources and the need for policy documents.considering dysphagia as only a postgraduate specialisation area was described by respondents as being impractical since all hospital speech therapists were faced with the ethical responsibility of treating dysphagia patients on a daily basis. in addition, it is recommended that supervision/mentoring systems be implemented using existing resources and expertise from both academic institutions and hospital departments. more than half (59%) of the respondents proposed strategies for dealing with the inadequacies of undergraduate training. it was felt that the course should be taught as a separate module which was structured to provide students with a firm theoretical grounding. significantly more emphasis on the practical hands-on training in dysphagia assessment and treatment was suggested with both normal subjects and dysphagia patients. respondents also felt that students should be involved in more clinical practicals at hospitals in order to gain experience in multidisciplinary dysphagia management. it was felt that students should also be taught about legal and ethical issues and be exposed to advances in instrumentation at an die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 n a i n a m o d i & e l e a n o r ross undergraduate level. in addition, respondents recommended a need for the university lecturers involved in teaching the area of dysphagia to be adequately experienced in the area, particularly o n a practical level. there was a strong consensus among respondents that all postgraduate courses include a practical component in a d d i t i o n to t h e theory. a s y s t e m of o b s e r v i n g s p e e c h t h e r a p i s t s at o t h e r h o s p i t a l s w a s also r e c o m m e n d e d . h o w e v e r , r e s p o n d e n t s d i d n o t d i s t i n g u i s h b e t w e e n postgraduate training for degree or non-degree purposes. in order to improve access to resources it was felt that support g r o u p s c o n s i s t i n g o f therapists from different hospitals would allow for case discussions and guidance. developing a specialist list would also allow for easy contact with more experienced therapists whenever the need arose. c o n c l u s i o n in conclusion, the results of this study suggest that many slps in s o u t h africa m a y b e i n v o l v e d in the area o f dysphagia intervention. findings have also indicated that there are possible gaps in clinicians' knowledge and skills in this area, which may be linked to inadequate training r e c e i v e d at u n d e r g r a d u a t e a n d p o s t g r a d u a t e l e v e l s , particularly with regard to practical hands-on training as well as theory o n advances in instrumentation; ethical issues; safety and emergency procedures; and multidisciplinary management of dysphagia. furthermore, clinicians are probably also faced with limited access to supervision systems as well as limited resources in general. these difficulties would appear to be further confounded by limited k n o w l e d g e o f t h e i r role in d y s p h a g i a b y o t h e r health professionals and the subsequent need to actively market their services in their work settings. these problems faced by clinicians, in addition to the life-threatening nature of the w o r k , h i g h l i g h t s the need for clinicians, t r a i n i n g institutions and the professional organisation for slps to make a concerted effort to improve the existing situation. however, possibly the most important reason to undertake such efforts is for the basic rights of dysphagia patients themselves. as health care workers, "the concept of quality care is fundamental to the values, ethics and traditions of our profession at the very least to do no harm; usually to do some good; and ideally to realise the greatest good possible in any given situation" [frattali, 1990, p.39]. note the first author is a senior speech and hearing therapist employed at the chris hani baragwanath hospital and conducted this research project as a partial requirement for the degree m.a. speech pathology at the university of the witwatersrand. references american speech-language-hearing association. (1985). clinical supervision in speech-language pathology and audiology. asha, 28 (6), 57-60. american speech-language-hearing association. (1987). ad hoc committee on dysphagia report. asha, 29 (4), 57-58. american speech-language-hearing association. (1989). knowledge and skills needed by speech-language patho-logists providing services to dysphagic patients. asha, 32 (suppl. 2), 7-12. american speech-language-hearing association. (1991). instrumental diagnostic procedures for swallowing. asha, 33,67-73. american speech-language-hearing association. (1993). preferred practice patterns for the professions of speech-language pathology and audiology. asha, 35 (suppl. 11), 1-100. american speech-language-hearing association. (1994). code of ethics. asha, 36 (suppl. 13), 1-2. australian association of speech and hearing. (1994). dysphagia position paper. australia: aash. bernthal, j.e. and bankson, n.w. (1992). higher education. in j.a. rassi and m.d. mcelroy (eds.) the education of audiologists and speech-language pathologists. maryland: york press. cannito, m.p. (1995). establishing a dysphagia curriculum at the university of memphis: a personal perspective. asha special interest divisions, 4 (2), 7-8. college of medicine of south africa. (1991). management of hiv positive patients. south african medical journal, 79, 688-690. college of speech therapists. (1990). position paper on dysphagia. u.k.: cst. frattali, c.m. (1990). quality assurance today: learning the basics. asha, 32, 39-40. groher, m.e. (1995). preparing the speech-language pathologist in dysphagia management: a serious problem with emerging solutions. asha special interest divisions, 4 (2), 2-3. kulpa, j.i. (1990). aids / hiv: implications for speech-language pathologists and audiologists. asha, 32, 46-48. langmore, s.e. (1991). managing the complications of aspiration in dysphagic adults. seminars in speech and language, 12 (3), 199-208. .logemann, j. (1995). education in dysphagia. asha special interest divisions, 4 (2), 3-5. logemann, j.a. (1998). evaluation and treatment of swallowing disorders. usa: pro-ed publishers. martin, b.j.w. (1995). dysphagia evaluation and management: clinical training, clinical competency and speciality recognition. the south african journal of communication disorders, 42, 3-6. mchorney, c.a. and rosenbek, j.c. (1998). functional outcome assessment of adults with oropharyngeal dysphagia. seminars in speech and language, 19 (3), 235-246. miller, r.m. (1995). aperspective on a dysphagia course: one man's view. asha special interest divisions, 4 (2), 5-7. mirro, j.f. and patey, c. (1991). developing a dysphagia dietary program. seminars in speech and language, 12 (3), 218-227. preliminary draft of policy document for communication professionals. (1993). s.a.: bonaledi nursing college. rassi, j.a. and mcelroy, m.d. (1992a). curriculum development. in j.a. rassi and m.d. mcelroy (eds.) the education of audiologists and speech-language pathologists. maryland: york press. rassi, j.a. and mcelroy, m.d. (1992b). education in the clinic', in j.a. rassi and m.d. mcelroy (eds.) the education of audiologists and speech-language pathologists. maryland: york press. sonies, b.c. (1991). instrumental procedures for dysphagia diagnosis. seminars in speech and language, 13 (3), 185-198. south african speech-language hearing association. (1996). newsletter. south africa: saslha. | south african speech-language hearingassociation. (1997). code of ethics. south africa: saslha. south african speech-language hearing association. (1998). guidelines on service provision in hospitals. south africa: saslha. ibner, m.a. (1995). obstacles to providing dysphagia training in a small academic program. asha special interest divisions, 4 (2), 8-9. white paper for the transformation of the health system in south africa. 16 april 1997. government gazette notice no. 667 of 1997. pretoria: government printers. young, e.c. and durant-jones, l. (1990). developing a dysphagia program in an acute care hospital: a needs assessment. dysphagia, 5, 159-165. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 27 place pitch discrimination and speech recognition in cochlear implant users johan j hanekom department of electrical and electronic engineering university of pretoria robert v shannon head: department of auditory implant and perception house ear institute los angeles, california, united states of america abstract the considerable variability in speech perception performance among cochlear implant patients makes it difficult to compare the effectiveness of different speech processing strategies. one result is that optimal individualized processor parameter setting is not always achieved. this paper investigates the relationship between place pitch discrimination ability and speech perception to establish whether pitch ranking could be used as an aid in better patient-specific fitting of processors. three subjects participated in this study. place pitch discrimination ability was measured and this information was used to design new channel to electrode allocations for each subject. several allocations were evaluated with speech tests with consonant, vowel and sentence material. it is shown that there is correlation between the perceptual pitch distance between electrodes and speech perception performance. the results indicate that pitch ranking ability might be used both as an indicator of the speech perception potential of an implant user and in the choice of better electrode configurations. opsomming die beduidende verskille in spraakherkenningsvermoe van kogleere-inplantpasiente bemoeilik die vergelyking van die effektiwiteit van verskillende spraakverwerkingsstrategiee. 'n gevolg is dat die individuele instelhng van spraakverwerkerparameters 'vir pasiente nie altyd optimaal gedoen word nie. hierdie artikel ondersoek die verband tussen plek-toonhoogtediskriminasie en spraakherkenning om te bepaal of toonhoogte-rangskikking nuttig is as hulpmiddel vir beter gebruiker-spesifieke passing van spraakverwerkers. drie proefpersone het aan hierdie ondersoek deelgeneem. plektoonhoogtediskriminasie is gemeet en die inligting meruit is gebruik vir die ontwerp van nuwe afbeeldings van elektrodes op kanale. verskeie afbeeldings is evalueer met spraaktoetse met vokaal-, konsonanten sinsmateriaal. daar word aangetoon dat daar korrelasie bestaan tussen toonhoogtediskriminasie en spraakherkenningsvermoe. die resultate wys dat toonhoogterangskikkingsvermoe gebruik kan word as beide 'n indikator vir die spraakherkenningspotensiaal van 'n kogleereinplantgebruiker en vir die beter keuse van elektrodekonfigurasies. key words: cochlear implants, multi-electrode stimulation, pitch discrimination, speech recognition, neural selectivity, perceptual distance. introduction two parameters, which influence the speech perception abilities of cochlear implant users, are the quality of spectral information and the quality of temporal information received by their electrically activated auditory systems. this paper focuses on the importance of spectral information. multiple-electrode stimulation is preferred in cochlear implants, because it is generally accepted that the tonotopic organization found along the length of the cochlea in the healthy auditory system is retained to some degree for electrical hearing. many research studies, including earlier work by eddington (1980) and a recent study by nelson, van tasell, schroder & soli (1995), have shown that electrodes stimulating the more basal areas of the cochlea result in higher perceived pitches (or sharper tonal quality) and stimulation closer to the apex results in lower perceived pitches. it would be natural to assume that tonotopic organization in electrical hearing is retained by multiple electrodes selectively stimulating discrete neural populations. however, the assumption that discrete neural populations can be activated is not always true. when electrodes are closely spaced, considerable overlap occurs in the neural populations excited by the stimulation current, a problem which was addressed by tbwnshend & white (1987). this die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 is the result of spread of electrical current in the biological medium of the cochlea. the implication is that if two electrodes stimulate the same neural population or overlapping neural populations, sound sensations elicited by the two stimuli might be confused or might even be indistinguishable. this reduces the number of independent channels of information that can be conveyed to the cochlear implant user's auditory system. it is important to realize that the number of independent channels of information is not equal to the number of electrodes. the question which presents itself then, is how much overlap in stimulation of auditory neurons does occur in the cochlea, and how important is this in the patient's ability to understand speech? although it is generally assumed to be true, is it really beneficial to have excitation of distinct neural populations? in other words, on the one hand it is assumed that multi-electrode implants perform well because they utilize the tonotopical organization of the cochlea, but on the other hand current spread inside the cochlea might defeat this purpose by having different electrodes stimulating identical neural populations. if many electrodes stimulate the same neural population, why do some patients perform so well on open set speech recognition tasks? a secondary question which follows is whether using simpler implants with a smaller number of electrodes (which in turn could potentially be cheaper and more reliable) would not perform as well as implants with many electrodes. the questions formulated above and related questions have been investigated by a number of researchers. the effect of the number of electrodes on speech perception performance has been addressed in lawson, wilson, zerbi & finley (1996). they found that by increasing the number of electrodes, speech perception performance is improved, but for as few as four to seven electrodes, speech performance levels are close to what can be achieved by using ten or twenty electrodes. busby, whitford, blarney, richardson & clark (1994) studied patients' abilities to rank electrode pitch as a function of stimulation mode of the nucleus1 implant device. (clark, tong & patrick (1990) give detailed descriptions of this device). as will be explained in the text to follow, the nucleus device can utilize different stimulation modes, which produce differences in the amount of current spread from the electrodes. busby et al. (1994) found that the ability to rank pitch for stimulation on a specific electrode (in other words, place pitch) was related to the mode of stimulation used (and thus the amount of current spread). nelson et al. (1995) studied the relationship between pitch ranking ability (or electrode ranking ability) and consonant perception in ten subjects using the nucleus cochlear implant. they found correlation between the consonant perception and pitch discrimination, but they found little correlation between recognition of consonants based on recognition of place cues and place pitch perception. however, they concluded that this might be related to the speech processing strategy not taking full advantage of the user's ability to do place pitch ranking. this paper addresses some of the questions mentioned above. specifically, we investigate the question of the reone of the most widely used cochlear implant devices is the nucleus, manufactured by cochlear pty limited in australia and by their united states subsidiary, cochlear corporation. johan j hanekom en robert v shannon lationship between speech perception performance and the stimulation of overlapping neural populations in the cochlea. the approach used is to find a measure of the amount of overlap among stimulated neural populations. this is accomplished by determining the amount of electrode confusion with a pitch discrimination experiment. an additional question, which we also address, is whether it is possible to improve speech discrimination with the correct design of electrode configuration. experimental approach one way to quantify the amount of overlap among neural populations stimulated by different electrodes, is to measure the amount of confusion between electrodes. we measure the pitch discrimination between electrodes as a measure of the amount of electrode confusion, which is then also a measure of the amount of overlap in the neural populations stimulated. this is in turn a measure of the amount of current spread resulting from electrical stimulation. the procedure we use is to compile a place pitch ranking matrix (or electrode discrimination matrix) which is transformed to perceptual distance values, as explained below. based on the pitch discrimination information, we then design various maps and evaluate the speech perception performance for these. we focus primarily on the relationship between pitch discrimination data and speech perception performance for various different maps in a single subject, while repeating only some of the tests in other subjects. in this respect our approach differs from that used by nelson et al. (1995). they compared speech perception abilities in ten subjects using their everyday maps2, and related this to the subjects' place pitch ranking abilities. however, they did not study the effect of using different maps in the same patient. the rationale for this would be to investigate to which dimensions of place pitch discrimination speech perception is related. the term dimensions refers to the fact that place pitch discrimination ability might be related to various physical electrode parameters, for example electrode spatial separation and current spread from the electrodes, but also to perceptual distance between electrodes. our study evaluates nine different maps in the same subject, to establish whether speech recognition performance is related to the perceptual distance between electrodes or to other dimensions of pitch discrimination ability. it will become clear in the description of the properties of the pitch discrimination abilities of each of the subjects why more maps were evaluated in one specific subject. the three subjects who participated included a good user, the term map used in this context, usually refers to the patient-specific settings that are made to the nucleus processor. a map is a table of values with the threshold and uncomfortable loudness stimulation current values for each electrode. the map also holds information on the specific frequency allocation table used. the frequency allocation table is a table specifying the filter cutoff frequencies used in the twenty channels of the nucleus speech processor. different frequency allocation tables are available. the software used in this study to program the nucleus device allows the user an extra option, namely to allocate multiple filter channels to the same electrode. when we refer to map in this article, we are actually referring to this filter-to-electrode allocation. the other parameters we used for the maps were from the subjects' everyday maps and remained unchanged throughout the experiments, unless noted otherwise. everyday map refers to the regular maps that the subjects used daily. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) place pitch discrimination and speech recognition fair user and a relatively poor user. we relate our findines to this observation. perceptual distance between stimuli is quantified by d', a asure often used in psychophysical studies to express perm ® a l distances in forced choice experiments. this measw a s u s e d in the context of pitch discrimination studies b^townshend e t a i (1987) and more recently by nelson et al (1995)· smaller values of d' indicate more confusion between stimuli. negative values of d' in the pitch discrimination experiment indicate pitch reversal. for no confusion between stimuli, the largest value of d' (3.29) is achieved. the d's can be calculated from signal detection theory as described in green & swets (1966). hacker & ratcliff (1979) tabulated the values of d' for a two alternative forced choice experiment such as is described here. we used a vowel test, a consonant test and a sentence test in the evaluation of speech perception ability with each map. in previous studies (for example nelson et al., 1995) consonant perception was used to assess speech perception ability. we included a vowel test, because vowels are recognized primarily by their formant structure (dubno & dorman, 1987) and as such their recognition should be dependent on the ability to activate discrete neural populations selectively. sentence material was included in the speech perception tests to evaluate open set speech understanding for the various different maps. the number of electrodes used in the maps in our experiments was six or seven throughout. the reason for this choice was that it was found that seven-electrode maps gave speech perception performance levels that allowed some play for the speech perception scores to improve or deteriorate. a study by lawson et al. (1996) showed that speech recognition demonstrated a rapid decline when the number of electrodes was lowered from seven to four to two and to one, and that with a seven-electrode map it is possible to get speech discrimination close to what can be achieved by ten or twenty-electrode maps. an advantage of using a reduced number of electrodes is that the stimulation rate of the nucleus processor increases. higher stimulation rates have proved to result in better speech recognition performance (wilson et al., 1991). a corollary of this study is to establish a procedure which could optimize an electrode map when only a small number of electrodes are used. fewer electrodes than the twenty available in the nucleus might be used for several reasons. other implants that use fewer electrodes are in cochlear implant users 29 available. it is also possible that only a small number of stimulation sites are available as a result of poor neural survival, or that a reduced number of electrodes are available because of electrode damage. another possible application for fewer electrodes would be a future lower cost device. the rest of this paper is discussed in two sections. in the first section, the pitch discrimination experiment is described and in the second section the results are used in the design of maps which are evaluated with speech perception experiments. pitch discrimination experiment methodology subjects three users of the nucleus cochlear implant participated in this study. all of them were users of the nucleus spectra speech processor. this processor implements the speak speech processing strategy, which is described in skinner et al. (1994). table 1 contains detailed information on the three subjects. electrode parameters all three subjects used the nucleus 22 electrode array (described in clark et al., 1990, p. 114), implanted into the scala tympani. the electrode bands in this array are separated by 0.75 mm. we refer to the two electrodes of a stimulation pair as the stimulation electrode and the return electrode. stimulation was always with current-balanced biphasic pulsatile waveforms, with the positive pulse preceding the negative pulse. the stimulation electrode was the electrode on which a positive first biphasic pulse could be measured if the other electrode was used as reference. the return electrode was always the more apical of a stimulation pair. the electrode numbering convention used in this paper is as follows: an electrode pair is referred to by the stimulation electrode number, and with the stimulation mode known (explained below), the return electrode is implicitly known. electrode 1 was the most basal electrode and electrode 20 was the most apical electrode used as stimulation electrode. the nucleus speech processor allows different stimulation modes. in bp mode3, the stimulation electrode and table 1. subject information for the three subjects who participated in this study. insertion depth refers to the number of electrode bands inside the cochlea. the first twenty-two electrodes are active. subject age gender age of onset of profound hearing loss time of implant use processor type insertion depth cause of deafness ewb 55 male 45 6 years spectra 27 trauma jem 39 male 35 4 years spectra 26 trauma rek 54 male 47 6 months spectra 22 progressive hearing loss 3 the abbreviation bp, for bipolar, is used throughout this paper. this is the standard abbreviation used in texts by cochlear pty limited to indicate the nucleus device's stimulation modes. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 the return electrode are adjoining. in b p + 1 mode one electrode separates the stimulation electrode and the return electrode: in b p + 2 mode two electrodes separate the ξ γ ™ t r ° d e a n d ; e t u r n e l e c t r ° d e simulation modeus up to bp+3 were used in this study. the symbol δε is used for electrode spatial separation. stimulus parameters mode of stimulation. for subject jem w t ^^ ^ e d a bp+1 everyday map, we used stimulation in bp, b p ^ and ms i z z 1 " a ? d l t l ° n 1° b p + 1 m o d e · t h e reasons for this will become clear in the discussions to follow aij^stimuli were current-balanced biphasic pulses posi ζ p γ / 1 1 " 8 ' ; s t l m u l a t l 0 n r a * e was 1000 puisespersecthe l e n e t h ^ ) f u e d u r a t i o n 200 microsecond . was 500 ms flnhsmg p r e s e n t a t i o n ° n a « ο electrode was 500 ms and no ramping was used. stimuli were pre ut below ι ο : ' z t t t l e v e l ° f s t l m u l a t above 50%, but below 80% of the dynamic range of the subject. acorn c o r d e d t h t , t n n e r a t e d ^ a p p r ° p r i a t e stimuli and " s cor l t f ^ / r " 0 " ! ? ' t h e s t i m u l i w e r e — d e d in me correct format to enable presentation directly to the internal receiver of the nucleus device. the coded sttmuli were p r e s e n t e d directly to the internal receiver οο γ ο the subjects (the subjects' processors were not used) ^ a a custom m t e r f a c e (described in shannon, adams fe^rel pal umbo & grandgenett, 1990). ' psychophysical procedure eact of t h 7 h d l s t m m a t l 0 n m a t r i x was measured for u s s f o r all th b p + 1 s t l m u l a t i o n o d e was u i p i n> κ i s u b j e c t s ' a n d i n a d d i t i ° n we also measured pitch discrimination matrices for bp, b p + 2 and β ρ λ mode for subject jem. + 3 the pitch discrimination matrix was compiled by usstirmiltof 500 p s y c h ° p h ^ p-cedure. consecutive stimuli of 500 ms, separated by a brief quiet interval of the s = e n w 0 n , t w ° ° f ^ s ^ c t ' s e l e s e piched r t t " t 0 j u d g e w h i c h s t i m u l u * was higher s a m tbl « w 3 s c ° n t r 0 l l e d ^ a computer p r o s z r ί ! γ h a d t 0 i n d i c a t e h i s c h o i c e by depressing th^firqt w o buttons, with one button c o r r e s p o n d to the first s o u n d and the other button to the second sound d e l ^ t s o m t t ° f l t l m u l 1 w a s p-sented x r a delay of 200 ms after the patient had made his choice ζ γ : ζ τ 0 η ΐ ο τ repetiti°n °f t h e p a i r o f < p n u 7 t p a i r c o n s i s t e d of stimuli on two different electrodes. the stimuli were balanced for loudness to minimize confusions between loudness and pitch thts was done b y l o u d b a l a n d n g o c e pteh.̂ ihiswas z z f p l t c t d l s c n m i n a t l 0 n experiments. a reference midd e ofth: c i t ' , 1 l s u a l l y e l e c t r ° d e 1 0 ' w h i c h » the middle of the electrode array. the subject was asked to potcatheeastir 0ttabl! " ΐ " ^ l 6 v e 1 ' a n d ^ ts pdur! pose tne stimulation levels were varî h , tween 50% and 80% of the d y z ^ t s ^ ot thereference stimulus being presented, followed by a w " r peated3 a ? " 1 1 6 e l e c t r ° d e s ^ ^mjns^ x e s e s f t t y 3 8 n e c e s s a ^ i n to find level setting for the second stimulus which had the same johan j hanekom en robert v shan apparent loudness as the first stimulus, l b be more a, ' rate in pinpointing the stimulation level which was " in loudness to the reference, subjects were also a s k ^ 1 find a level that was just noticeably softer than the r r 1 0 ence and also to find a level just not.eab y loude/th^' the reference^ in this way three datapoints z e l t hshed for each electrode's simulation level and we j ' able to make a good estimation of the stimulus m r ! ' 6 necessary to have al, the electrode s t i m u ^ ^ * the electrodes used for the stimulation pairs duri the pitch discrimination experiment were complem8 randomized for each run. one run consisted οί γ ρ ^ tation of all possible combinations of electrodes fn l "' p w r , o . e s " \ o r m o d e · this amounted to twentv sion between electrodes, a considerable n t m b e r of c l pansons were needed. twenty runs were completed tn b p + 1 mode for each of the subjects, which gave a t o s of forty comparisons of each electrode with evfry other elel to ϊ ί ΐ r e a c t i ° n ' i n d i c a t i n g which stimulus was judged mmssszt electrode of a stimulation pair was iudwh f„ ι· , pitched than the « o r e a p i c a u w h r x t u k tte e m order based o „ the tonotopic organization rftie s f a ) a p , « l eieetade to be higher pitched c x l ™ ' ^ re s d ° i f a t " " 1 ; ρ * γ · w m ™ d e s o n d very aistinct pitch difference, resulting in a l a m a' without the pitch difference being very w e a large d , probably an indication of w h e t h e / o v e l p p f g or ^ neural populations are stimulated n u m h ^ f p i d t y w a s p r e s e n t > electrodes with lower numbers (located more basally) would be expected t 7 z consistently judged higher in /u expected to be higher m i m w ! 7 f ! p t c h t h a n electrodes with companson i u n t ^ ^ ^ e l e c t r o d e s ) in a paired tfonst™ , ' l f c o m p l e t e l y separate neural populations (non-overlapping) were stimulated by each electrode i e t t r t c l ° s e e l e c t r ° d e s p a d n ^ ' n ° c o ^ u s t n b e t e s electrodes was expected. on the other hanh 7 s m f a s s k w f t i f f l a f e s . s a n ^ c a t , o n of the amount of current the south africa journal of communication disorders, vol. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lace tc discrimination and speech recognitionin cochlear implant users 31 results t i m u l u s r e s p o n s e matrices for the three subjects t h e s in fi srures 1 to 4 after they have been converted a r e shown i " " 6 1 0.36 0.62 2 -0.07 3 1 0.36 0.62 2 -0.07 1 0.36 0.62 2 -0.07 0.18 0.36 0,51 .0.36 0.25 0.62 0.00 -0.62 -0.18 0.43 0.18 0.36 0,51 .0.36 0.25 0.62 0.00 -0.62 -0.18 0.43 0.07 •0.18 0.18 0.36 0,51 .0.36 0.25 0.62 0.00 -0.62 -0.18 0.43 0.07 •0.18 0.00 0.07 1.53 1.66 .0.25 0.62 1 53 0.00 -0.62 -0.18 0.43 0.07 •0.18 0.00 0.07 1.53 1.66 .0.25 0.62 1 53 0.00 -0.62 -0.18 0.43 0.62 1.00 0.51 0.07 1.53 1.66 1.81 2.09 7 33 1 53 1.53 1.35 1.14 0.25 0.07 1.53 1.66 1.81 2.09 7 33 2.33 2.33 2.33 1.24 1.66 0.36 1 81 3.29 3.29 1.81 1.66 2.09 1.66 1.24 0.91 2.90 3.29 3.29 2.90 2.90 3.29 2.33 1.81 1.53 0.51 3?9 2.90 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.90 2.90 2.09 3.29 1.81 1.66 1.14 190 2.90 3.29 3.29 2.33 2.90 3.29 3.29 1.81 1.66 1.81 1.24 0.91 3.29 3.29 3->9 3 29 3.29 2.90 3.29 3.29 2.90 2.90 2.33 1.66 2.33 1.66 1.14 3.29 3.29 3->9 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.90 2.90 3.29 2.90 2.09 1.53 1.35 1.14 3 79 3 29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.90 3.29 2.90 1.53 2.33 2.33 1.81 1.81 0.18 3.29 1.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.90 2.90 3.29 2.90 2.09 2.90 3.29 1.66 1.53 1.66 1.00 0.91 3.29 3.29 3.29 1.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.33 3.29 2.33 3.29 2.90 3.29 3.29 2.09 2.90 1.66 1.24 1.14 0.62 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.90 2.90 2.33 2.33 2.09 2.09 2.09 1.14 0.78 0.70 0.36 figure 1. pitch discrimination matrix for subject jem for bp+1 mode of stimulation. positive values of d' in the table indicate that the electrode numbered at the top was judged to have a higher pitch than the electrode numbered at the left, d' values of 3.29 indicate 100% consistency in pitch judgement and values higher than 1.5 indicate an 85% consistency. jem had a large area of good pitch discrimination. 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 0.18 1.19 0.74 0.00 0.18 -0.74 0.18 0.74 -0.55 0.54 0.54 0.18 -0.74 0.18 0.18 > 1.19 0.18 0.18 0.74 0.95 0.36 [ 1.81 1.47 1.47 2.33 1.81 1.47 0.95 1 2.33 3.29 1.81 2.33 3.29 1.81 2.33 1.47 1 3.29 2.33 2.33 3.29 3.29 2.33 2.33 2.33 0.74 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.33 3.29 1.81 1.81 0.95 2.33 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.33 3.29 1.81 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 1.19 1.47 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 1.47 0.18 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.33 2.33 1.81 1.47 0.18 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 1.81 0.74 0.36 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 1.19 1.19 0.18 0.36 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 3.29 2.33 1.47 3.29 2.33 0.95 0.95 0.54 0.18 figure 2. pitch discrimination matrix for subject jem for bp+3 stimulation mode. note that the number of available electrodes was only 18 instead of 20. that is because the return electrode, for electrode 18 as stimulation electrode in bp+3 mode, is electrode band 22, which is the most basal electrode band. also, note that the pitch discrimination pattern differs from that in figure 1. some electrodes which did not exhibit good discrimination in bp+1 mode, had good discrimination in bp+3 mode. compare the d' between electrodes 11 and 12 in these stimulation modes. the inverse was also true for some electrodes. ' to d' values. two aspects are immediately evident from these matrices. first, the distribution of areas where electrode discrimination was better, varied considerably for the three subjects. this might have been dependent on electrode placement, with areas of good electrode discrimination being where the electrodes were situated closer to the (surviving) nerves. this result underlines the fact that there are two important but uncontrollable factors in perception of sound in cochlear implants: (1) placement of electrode and (2) nerve survival. if a d' of 1.5 was (arbitrarily) taken as a criterion for largely independent neural populations (this corresponds 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 0.36 0.36 0.36 1.47 0.95 0.54 0.74 0.74 -0.36 0.00 1.19 1.19 1.81 0.18 0.18 0.95 1.47 1.81 0.74 0.36 0.18 1.19 0.95 0.74 0.00 0.55 0.74 0.18 0.54 0.18 0.36 0.00 1.47 0.95 1.19 0.74 1.47 1.19 1.47 0.54 0.00 0.74 0.00 0.54 1.47 1.81 2.33 2.33 1.19 1.47 0.95 0.18 0.54 1.47 0.54 2.33 1.81 1.19 1.81 0.74 0.54 1.19 1.19 1.19 0.74 0.36 3.29 3.29 1.81 1.19 1.47 1.19 0.95 0.74 3.29 0.74 0.18 0.18 2.33 3.29 2.33 3.29 0.54 1.19 0.74 0.54 1.47 0.18 0.18 0.54 0.00 2.33 3.29 1.81 1.81 0.95 1.19 1.19 1.47 1.81 0.54 0.36 0.54 0.36 •0.36 3.29 2.33 1.81 1.81 1.81 0.54 0.18 1.81 1.47 0.74 0.36 0.36 0.00 0.18 0.00 3.29 0.95 1.81 2.33 0.36 0.36 0.54 0.74 1.47 1.19 0.36 0.18 0.36 0.18 •0.36 0.36 1.47 0.00 0.36 0.36 •0.36 •0.18 •0-18 0.00 0.74 •0.55 0.95 1.47 -0.74 •0.18 -0.74 •1.19 0.95 0.18 •0.74 «.36 0.55 •0.18 •1.47 •1.47 •1.47 0.95 •2.33 •1.81 1.47 •1.19 1.81 •1.47 -1.81 0.00 0.55 1.19 0.55 0.36 •0.18 •0.18 1.19 0.74 •0.36 0.55 0.95 •1.19 •1.47 1.19 1.81 1.19 •1.47 •1.81 •0.36 1.47 figure 3. pitch discrimination matrix for subject rek. the values in the table are the calculated d' values. positive values of d' indicate that the electrode numbered at the top was judged higher in pitch than the electrode numbered at the left more than half of the time. for normal tonotopic organization, the d' values are expected to be positive, as electrode 1 is the most basal electrode and electrode 20 the most apical. rek had a region of good pitch discrimination toward the left side of the matrix. ι 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 ii 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 -0.07 -0.70 0.00 0.14 -0.14 0.00 0.54 -0.07 0.62 0.54 -0.21 0.07 -0.21 0.21 0.14 -0.43 -0.14 -0.07 -0.07 -0.43 0.36 0.29 -0.07 -0.21 -0.21 -0.21 -0.21 -0.14 0.14 -0.29 0.62 0.21 -0.07 0.00 0.07 -0.14 0.87 -0.14 0.36 •0.07 0.43 0.62 0.07 0.29 0.36 0.54 0.21 0.87 0.07 0.14 0.00 0.21 0.43 0.43 -0.14 0.78 0.29 0.70 0.14 -0.14 0.70 0.14 0.21 0.29 0.36 -0.07 0.36 0.00 0.54 0.07 0.21 0.54 0.36 0.54 0.54 -0.14 -0.36 0.00 0.29 -0.21 0.21 -0.21 0.00 0.07 0.21 0.21 0.14 -0.43 0.00 -0.62 -0.29 0.43 -0.07 0.07 0.14 -0.36 0.14 0.07 0.00 0.14 0.07 -0.78 -0.43 0.14 0.21 0.54 -0.21 0.00 -0.36 0.00 -0.43 -0.14 0.07 -0.70 -0.70 -0.36 -0.87 -0.95 -0.36 0.07 0.95 0.54 1.14 0.62 0.70 0.70 0.87 0.87 0.54 0.21 0.70 0.07 0.29 1.05 0.78 1.05 1.05 0.78 0.87 1.24 0.87 0.87 0.62 1.35 0.95 0.70 0.70 0.70 0.29 0.78 1.14 1.24 0.43 1.66 0.78 1.14 1.14 1.14 1.35 0.95 1.99 1.35 0.70 1.05 0.95 0.78 1.24 1.05 1.81 1.05 -0.07 1.14 1.47 1.05 1.05 1.05 1.66 1.35 1.47 0.95 1.24 1.24 0.87 1.05 1.66 1.24 1.47 1.14 1.14 0.78 figure 4. pitch discrimination matrix for subject ewb. this subject exhibited poor pitch discrimination throughout the electrode array, although he did a little bit better toward the apical side. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 to 85% consistency in pitch ranking), then subject ewb had almost no discrimination between any of his electrodes, although he did a little better toward the apical side. subject rek had a region of good pitch discrimination, but primarily for large electrode separations. subject jem had very good electrode discrimination throughout his electrode array when the electrode spatial separation was two or more. his best region was near to the middle of his electrode array. more details concerning the analysis of the properties of the subjects' pitch discrimination matrices are discussed when the maps used in the speech perception experiment are described below. speech perception experiment methodology objective the speech perception experiment was used to investigate whether there was correlation between subjects' ability to discriminate among electrodes based on place pitch (as reflected by the information in the pitch discrimination matrices), and speech perception performance. two parameters (which could be deduced from the pitch discrimination matrices) were identified as being important in speech perception: the perceptual distance between pitch sensations elicited by electrodes as reflected by the d' values and δε, the electrode spatial separation. the maps used where chosen to reflect the effect on speech perception of varying these two parameters. speech materials we used vowel, consonant and sentence material to evaluate speech perception performance. all the speech material was available on laser disk. the vowel test used a set of eight vowels in a /hvdi context uttered by a male talker. each vowel syllable was repeated three times in randomized order. the consonant test used sixteen consonants in a /aca/ context. the consonant syllables were repeated five times in randomized order during a single consonant test. the sentence test consisted of a set of thirty-six sentences uttered by a female speaker. no repetitions of the same sentences were used and a new set of sentences was used for the evaluation of every map. none of the subjects had been tested with the specific sentence material before. procedure the software used for the creation of the subject maps allowed the programmer to allocate the output of any of the nucleus processor's filters to any electrode. multiple filter outputs could be allocated to the same electrodes. this enabled us to use maps utilizing a reduced set of electrodes, while still presenting all the spectral information from the twenty filters to the electrodes. the operation of the nucleus implant is such that reducing the number of electrodes increased the stimulation rate on the electrodes that were used (shannon et al., 1990). subjects were tested with their regular bp+1 maps and frequency allocation tables which they had been using daily johan j hanekom en robert v shannon for several months. the only changes to these maps were that the regular filter-to-electrode allocations were replaced by maps with a reduced number of electrodes of which multiple filter channels were allocated to each electrode. the specific choices of electrodes which were used in the various different maps are explained below. the subjects wore these experimental maps for a period of two full days before evaluation with the speech material. tests were conducted in a sound-isolated booth. speech stimuli were presented at 60 db spl. speech stimuli were played from a laser disc player through high quality audio loudspeakers. a computer program controlled the presentation of the speech material to the subjects. the subjects responded by indicating their choice on a computer keyboard. the computer program recorded the subject reaction for the vowel and consonant tests, and compiled stimulus-response matrices for these. the computer program also controlled the laser disc player for presentation of the sentences. the subject had to repeat as many words as he could understand from the sentence material, which was then recorded by the experimenter. map parameters details of the maps that were used for each patient are explained below. as explained earlier, all maps were sevenelectrode maps, except two of jem's maps, which were six-electrode bp+3 maps. maps were chosen to give either large or small cumulative values of d', and maps with similar cumulative d' were evaluated using different electrode spatial separations. both orderly and disorderly electrode spatial separations were used. table 2 summarizes the maps used. the motivation for the choice of each of the maps is given in the descriptions below. we evaluated more maps for jem than for the other two subjects. this was primarily dictated by the fact that subject jem's pitch discrimination matrix provided more degrees of freedom in the choice of various maps. this statement will become clear in the explanation of the procedure used to choose electrodes to be used in the maps. this procedure was very simple and was as follows: a list of all possible combinations forming seven-electrode maps was compiled (for example electrodes 1, 2, 3, 4, 5 6 7 or electrodes 1, 3, 5, 7, 9, 11, 13 and so on): then, 'for each subject, the cumulative d' was calculated for all these electrode sets by simply adding the corresponding six d' values. for example for subject rek, for the electrode set consisting of electrodes 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6 and 7, the six d' values to be added were 0.36 (d' between electrode 1 and 2), 0.36 (the d' between electrode 2 and 3), -0.36, 0, 0.18 and 0.18. the cumulative d' was thus calculated as 0.36. it was assumed that electrodes were distinguishable if d' was larger than 1.5. from subject rek's pitch discrimination matrix in figure 2, it can be seen that only a small region in the lower left corner of this matrix produced d' values in the region of 1.5 or larger, and then for an electrode spacing of four or larger. for smaller a criterion level of d'>l, a larger area in the d' matrix was identified. between electrodes 9 and 10 a d' of 1.47 was produced but for all other electrodes the electrode spatial separation had to be at least three to find a d'larger than 1. it can also be seen that electrodes could not be discriminated at the apical end of the electrode array. in this area all electrodes were confused and d' values were generally very low this means that to find a seven-electrode set with a large value the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lace pitch discrimination and speech recognitionin cochlear implant users 33 of cumulative d', the electrodes had to be chosen more toward the basal side of the array. even then we would not be able to choose an electrode set with all d' values greater than 1. for example, if we started with electrode 1, the next electrode had to be electrode 5 for d' greater than 1. then, the third electrode had to be electrode 11 to have a d' greater than 1 between electrode 5 and the third electrode to be used. from electrode 11 toward the apical side of the electrode array, all electrodes were confused. comparing this to jem's pitch discrimination matrix, it becomes clear that for jem there was much more flexibility in possible choices of electrodes for good cumulative d'. a d' of greater than one was found for all electrodes if electrode spatial separation was eight, and for most electrodes if the spatial separation was only two or larger, excluding electrodes to the more basal side of the array. this gave us many different possibilities of choice of seven-electrode maps with good cumulative d'. confusion between electrodes occurred primarily on the basal electrodes. there seemed to be total confusion of electrode pitch for all electrodes in subject ewb's case, except for a few electrodes near the basal end of the array, l b find a d' of larger than 1, electrode spatial separation had to be at least 14 in the more apical half of the electrode array, and no d's of 2 or larger were found. this gave very little flexsubject: jem map no electrodes used stim mode d' c δε a d no of sites vowel test cons test sent test 1 3,4,9,13,15,17,19 bp+1 10.3 2.7 2.2 5 92 88 95 2 6,7,8,9,10,11,12 bp+1 2.54 1 1 1 54 76 20 3 2,4,6,9,11,17,2 bp+1 4.98 3 2 2 88 88 88 4 4,5,10,11,16,17,19 bp+1 7.46 2.5 4 3 79 83 85 5 3,4,9,13,15,17 bp+3 6.53 2.8 3 3 83 89 92 6 2,4,6,9,11,17 bp+3 7.27 3 2.3 4 79 85 82 7 12,13,14,15,16,17,18 bp+1 5.42 1 1 1 62 81 66 8 2,5,8,11,14,17,20 (bp,bp,bp2,bp2,bp2, bp2,bp1) j mixed 13+ 3 1 5 92 90 97 9 2,4,7,10,13,16,jl9 bp+1 6.64 2.8 1.1 3 88 85 98 subject: rek map no electrodes used i 1 stim mode d' c δε a d no of sites vowel test cons test sent test 1 1,3,6,10,12,14,116 . bp+1 4.37 2.5 1.8 2 46 56 38 2,-, 1,2,5,12,13,14,19 bp+1 0.60 3 5 1 58 65 74 3 2,4,6,8,10,12,14 bp+1 2.00 2 1 1 50 55 38 4 2,5,8,11,14,17,20 bp+1 -0.87 3 1 1 46 88 94 5 1,4,7,10,13,16,19 bp+1 0.41 3 1 1 67 74 87 subject: ewb map no electrodes used stim mode d' c δε a d no of sites vowel test cons test sent test 1 3,5,8,16,17,18,20 bp+1 3.72 2.8 3.25 1 38 49 30 2 1,4,7,9,12,14,16 bp+1 -0.55 2.5 1.5 1 62 53 33 3 1,4,7,10,13,16119 bp+1 0.86 3 1 1 67 49 57 table 2. results for the speech perception experiment. the number/) is a simple measure for the disorder in electrode spacing as calculated by equation 1. δελ is the average spacing between electrodes. d'c is the cumulative d'. no of sites refer to the number of discrete stimulation sites when d ' > 1.5 is used as criterion. cons and sent are abbreviations of consonant and sentence, respectively. the table lists the stimulation modes used for each electrode for the mixed stimulation mode map for jem (map 8). the abbreviations bp1 and bp2 are used for bp+1 and bp+2 stimulation modes, respectively. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 johan j hanekom en robert v shannon ibility in choice of electrodes if a seven-electrode map was to be constructed, because although a range of different cumulative values for d' could be found, the electrode pitch sensations could not be discriminated for many of the possible electrode pairs. the hypothesis we wanted to test was whether electrodes that were clearly distinguishable were better choices to achieve good speech perception performance. at least we wanted to be able to compare good and poor electrode discrimination. consequently, only three different maps were tested for subject ewb. two choices of cumulative d' were contrasted: a map with ewb's best possible cumulative d' (3.72), and a map with a very poor cumulative d' of -0.55 were used. map 2 (with the poorer cumulative d') had the electrodes spaced more orderly than map 1, with δε at least 2. δε was 2 toward the apex and 3 toward the base. map 1 had a very disorderly electrode spacing with a big gap between electrodes 8 and 16 and some electrodes having spatial separation of only 1. the third map had an intermediate value of cumulative d' and all δε were 3 throughout. five maps were tested for subject rek. map 1 utilized both a good cumulative d' and a reasonably good electrode spacing. although the electrode spacing was irregular, δε was equal to or larger than 2. the cumulative d' of 4.37 was close to the best cumulative d' of 5.12 achievable for rek. the second map had a small cumulative d' of only 0.6, but with the interesting property that the d' values between every second electrode were relatively large. this map possibly stimulated only four distinct sites in the cochlea. map 3 had a cumulative d' of 2, which was near 50 % of the maximum achievable d', with a very orderly electrode spacing of 2. the electrodes used were situated in the middle of the electrode array. maps 4 and 5 also used very orderly electrode spacings, but this time the spatial separations used were 3. the cumulative d's were the small value of-0.87 and 0.41 respectively. nine maps were tested for subject jem. these maps tested speech perception performance for a spectrum of cumulative d's, from very low to the highest achievable values for this subject. some of these maps had very orderly electrode spacing, and some of them had very irregular electrode spacing. six of the maps used bp+1 mode (which was the stimulation mode used in jem's everyday map). two of the maps used bp+3 mode and one used different stimulation modes on different electrodes. map 1 was chosen for maximum d' (10.34) in bp+1 mode. the electrode spacing was irregular with the largest electrode spatial separation of 5 between electrodes 4 and 9. map 2 was chosen for low d' (2.54) and had an electrode spacing of 1. map 3 had an irregular spacing with a large separation gap between electrodes 11 and 17. the map was chosen for its cumulative d' of 4.98, in the middle of the d' value of maps 1 and 2. map 4 was chosen to be similar to map 2'of subject rek. although this map had a large cumulative d' of 7.46, electrodes were grouped into pairs, with the spacing between the pairs being only 1, and these electrodes were essentially stimulating the same areas within the cochlea. map 5 en map 6 used exactly the same electrodes as maps 1 and 2 respectively, but bp+3 stimulation mode was used. these maps were chosen to test whether the wider spread of stimulation current in bp+3 mode would influence the speech results negatively. thus, if d' was a good indication of current spread, then larger current spread would result in smaller d' values and poorer speech perception performance. map 7, a bp+1 map, tested another δε = 1 map, this time closer to the base, where jem had better electrode discrimination. map 8 was an attempt to create a map with the maximum achievable d', with the best possible electrode spacing. different stimulation modes were used on different electrodes. the stimulation modes were chosen to give the best achievable d's. to obtain the d's for the other stimulation modes, pitch discrimination matrices were compiled for these. the cumulative d' for this multi-stimulation mode map was larger than 13. the exact value is not known, because the d's between electrodes 5 and 8, and also between electrodes 17 and 20, were unknown because the electrodes in these pairs used different stimulation modes for each electrode. map 9 was a δε = 3 map with an intermediate value of d'. results although it was clear that electrode spatial separation was a parameter in determining speech perception performance, we had to find a way to quantify this parameter. various simple measures of how well the electrodes were spaced, were used. we calculated a value for the average δε, δεβ, by simply adding all the inter-electrode separations and dividing by the number of inter-electrode separations (six for a seven-electrode map). we also calculated a number, d, for the amount of electrode disorder caused by using irregular electrode spacing. the rationale for using this measure of disorder was that when we used these seven-electrode maps, we always added the same filter outputs, but for each choice of map these filter outputs were relayed to a different electrode. in some map we connected two neighbouring filters to electrodes that were spaced far apart. thus, we were actually introducing spectral distortion, and the number d calculated for electrode disorder gave some indication of how much this distortion was. a third measure of the quality of our choice of electrode spacing was to find a measure for the number of discrete stimulation sites activated. this could be found from the d's under the assumption that the perceptual distance between two electrodes was an indication of how much overlap there was in neural excitation. we assumed that a d' of 1.5 reflected reasonably little overlap, because this value of d' translated to an 85% consistency in pitch judgements. | table 2 summarizes the results for all the maps tested for the three subjects. the electrodes used in each map, the cumulative d's and the three measures reflecting the electrode spatial separations are included in the table. the results are given as percentage correct scores for tlie vowel, consonant and sentence tests. several interesting observations can be made from these results when we investigate the relative contribution of the two parameters (d' and electrode spacing) to speech perception performance. simple linear regression analysis was used to relate various map parameters to the results of the speech perception tests. regression lines and correlation coefficients are given in the figures. significance o f correlation was tested by the t-test and a 5% level of significance was used. speech perception as a function of cumulative d' as explained earlier, perceptual pitch distance as measured by d' was used as measure of the amount of overlap the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) place pitch discrimination and speech recognitionin cochlear implant users 35 in neural population stimulated by electrodes. for a specific seven-electrode map, cumulative d' is also simply a measure of a subject's ability to pitch rank this set of electrodes. figures 5 to 7 relate speech perception results to this measure for each of the subjects. the three figures for each subject give results for vowels, consonants and sentences, respectively. as was also demonstrated by nelson et al. (1995), significant correlation between pitch ranking ability and speech perception performance was evident. there was, however, considerable variation in how pitch ranking ability was correlated with speech perception. the regression lines and correlation coefficients for linear regression are given in the figures. in general, for subject jem, for whom the most data were available, higher cumulative values of d' were correlated to better performance on the three speech perception measures. there was a correlation of around 70% for all three speech perception measures to cumulative d'. there was a significant increase in vowel perception with larger values of d', the total range being nearly 40%. consonant perception increased over a smaller range of 14%. far too few data points were available for subject ewb to be conclusive. no correlation between vowel perception and cumulative d' was evident (r=0.03), but there was correlation with consonant perception (r=0.95) and sentence perception (r=0.68). the vowel test did not have significant correlation to the cumulative d ' for subject rek. the weakest vowel performance was achieved for the smallest and the largest values of d'. this subject showed an inverse relationship between cumulative d' and consonant scores. the best consonant recognition scores were achieved for small values of d'; increasing the cumulative d' decreased consonant perception. the correlation coefficient of 0.84 is significant. the range of consonant performance scores was υ cd « β cd eg e 1.5. based on this criterion, both rek and ewb had no or very little discrimination between different electrodes in the maps used. figure 11 shows the speech perception performance as a function of the number of electrode sites for jem. all the measures of speech performance showed significant correlation (r=0.7 or larger) with the number of discrete stimulation sites. speech perception as a function of position of electrodes most of the maps for subject jem were spread over the entire range of electrodes, except his map 2 and map 7. map 2 was in the middle of the electrode array, and map 7 was on the apical end of the electrode array. vowel and consonant scores improved slightly with the electrodes in the more apical position, but the sentence score improved dramatically from 20% to 66%. no similar data were measured for the other two subjects. speech perception as a function of the stimulation mode subject jem was the only subject for whom speech perception tests were conducted as a function of stimulation mode. maps 1 and 3 in bp+1 mode were repeated in bp+3 mode. the goal was to evaluate the effect of larger current distribution on speech perception. current spread is larger in bp+3 mode than in bp+1 mode (busby et al., 1994), therefore it was assumed that neural selectivity decreased and it was expected that speech perception performance would decrease. the bp+3 maps utilized the same electrodes as the corresponding bp+1 maps, except that the.most basal electrode iri each map was omitted. this decreased the number of discrete stimulation sites for map 5 (a bp+3 map) in comparison to map 1 (a bp+1 map), but, contrary to expectation, the number of discrete stimulation sites increased for map 6, the bp+3 counterpart for map 3 (a bp+1 map). for both maps significant (10%) decreases in vowel perception scores were observed. this might be explained by the bp+3 maps using only six instead of seven electrodes. an alternative explanation might be the fact that vowels are primarily recognized by their formant structure, and for the bp+3 mode the formant structure was less pronounced. however, there is no significant decrease in performance for the consonant and sentence tests. discussion the relationship between speech perception and pitch discrimination ability of subjects the experiments clearly identified both physical electrode spacing and perceptual electrode distance (as reflected by the cumulative d') as parameters determining speech perception performance. (note that pitch ranking ability as measured by cumulative d' is dependent on the specific set of electrodes used in a map.) these two parameters are related: the physical electrode spacing influences the amount of current distribution from electrodes. this in turn determines the neural selectivity that can be achieved. the actual number of discrete neural sites that are activated is also a function of other factors, including electrode distance from neurons, neural survival (zimmermann, burgess & nadol, 1995) and the conductive properties of the biological medium. thus, electrode spacing is vowels r =0.79 consonants r = 0.70 sentences r = 0.72 number of discrete stimulation sites figure 11. speech perception as a function of the number of discrete stimulation sites for subject jem. linear regression lines are fitted through the data and correlation coefficients are given above each panel. the number of discrete sites were calculated for each map by taking a d ' value of larger than 1.5 as criterion for two electrodes stimulating discrete neural populations. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) place pitch discrimination and speech recognitionin cochlear implant users 39 nhysical parameter determining electrical current 3 read. on the other hand, perceptual distance is a psychophysical parameter, meaning that this is a derived ° rameter, dependent on neural selectivity as well as experimenta'l and subject-dependent variables. thus, although there is correlation between current distribution and neural selectivity, the relationship is subject-dependent this is demonstrated by comparing the neural selectivity for bp+3 and bp+1 modes for jem, as reflected by the d' matrices in figures 1 and 2. the results also indicate that better overall pitch discrimination ability (measured over the set of all twenty electrodes) is related to better speech perception ability. this is demonstrated by comparing overall pitch discrimination ability to speech perception abilities. one measure of overall place pitch discrimination ability of a subject is the maximum cumulative d ' achievable for a specific number of electrodes. in this study, jem achieved a maximum seven-electrode cumulative d' of 10.34 in bp+1 mode. this can be compared to a maximum achievable cumulative d' of just more than 5 for rek and 3.72 for ewb. relating this to speech perception abilities as reflected by this study, jem was the best implant user in the group and ewb derived the least benefit from his implant. the relationship between the amount of benefit that the user derives from the implant and the pitch discrimination ability can also be quantified with two other measures of overall pitch discrimination ability. these are (1) the area of the d' matrix with large d' values (say d'>1.5) and (2) the minimum δε required for electrode discrimination. if d'>1.5 is used as the criterion for electrode discriminability, it is seen that jem generally required a δε of 2, while rek required a δε of 4 or more and ewb required large δε values to have d'>1.5. speech perception and spectral distortion disorder in the choice of electrode spacing did not seem to be an important parameter determining speech perception performance. however, the results obtained by shannon et al. (1995) suggested that spectral distortion might be an important factor determining cochlear implant users' speech perception abilities. an explanation for this apparent anomaly might be that different current distributions from each electrode, overlap in neural excitation and neural survival patterns might already introduce so much spectral distortion that the effect of poor electrode allocation is simply swamped. ' speech perception and the number of discrete neural sites although the number of discrete neural excitation sites in the cochlea (therefore, the number of discrete spectral locations) is correlated to speech perception performance, other information in the speech signal may also be utilized to understand electrical speech. this was evident from results achieved by ewb and rek. although being poorer users than jem, both demonstrated significant speech perception even with the excitation of so few discrete neural sites. these subjects might rely on temporal information for speech recognition. this paper emphasised the importance of cochlear place information (spectral information), but other studies have demonstrated the importance of temporal information in speech recognition (see, for example, shannon, zeng, kama'th, wygonski & ekelid (1990) or dorman et al. (1990)). using pitch ranking data to determine better subject-specific maps it is possible to make better choices of electrodes to be used in a reduced electrode map by basing the electrode choices on pitch discrimination data. the choice of a smaller number of electrodes leads to improved stimulation rates in the nucleus processor (shannon et al., 1990). it has been shown that higher stimulation rates lead to better speech perception performance (wilson et al., 1991). also, the careful choice of electrodes can be used to choose the set of electrodes with the best neural selectivity. the best neural selectivity is achieved by paying attention to two parameters: the physical electrode spacing, which determines current spread, and perceptual distance as measured with the d's, which is related to neural selectivity. furthermore, current distribution can be controlled by choice of stimulation mode. different stimulation modes result in different pitch ranking ability, as demonstrated in subject jem and also by busby et al. (1994). stimulation modes with larger current spread do not necessarily lead to decreased neural selectivity, as was demonstrated by map 6 of subject jem, where more discrete neural channels were achieved than in the bp+1 equivalent of this map. it might be possible to achieve better neural selectivity by. using multi-stimulation mode maps. by varying both electrode spacing and stimulation mode, current distribution patterns might be obtained which give larger perceptual distances between electrodes. this was accomplished in map 8 for subject jem. this technique for individualized fitting is more useful when subjects have good pitch discrimination abilities. for subjects in which pitch discrimination ability is not very good in a specific stimulation mode, other stimulation modes might be used. for subjects with poor pitch discrimination ability it might be advantageous to use fewer electrodes in order to increase the stimulation rate. the rationale is that if discrete excitation sites are few, place pitch abilities will be poor regardless of the number of electrodes used, and place pitch resolution may be substituted with better temporal resolution by increasing the stimulation rate. conclusions (1) the ability to discriminate electrodes based on place pitch, varies considerably among subjects. also, if it is assumed that perceptual distance between electrodes is related to current spread in the cochlea, the patterns of current spread vary considerably among subjects. (2) the variability in performance among subjects makes it difficult to compare the effectiveness of different speech processing strategies. also, there is a need for alternative procedures for better individualized fitting of processors. our results indicate that pitch ranking ability might be used both to assess implant user potential and to choose better electrode configurations. (3) two parameters that can be related to subjects' ability to rank pitch according to place of stimulation influence speech perception performance: electrode spatial ie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 j o h a n j h a n e k o m e n r o b e r t v shannon separation and perceptual distance between electrodes. (4) it is possible to make better choices of electrodes to be used in a reduced electrode m a p b y basing the electrode choices on pitch discrimination data. (5) it might be possible to achieve better neural selectivity b y using multi-stimulation mode maps. a c k n o w l e d g e m e n t s this research was done in the department of auditory implant and perception at the house ear institute in los angeles, u s a . we especially wish to thank kim fishman for m a k i n g available unpublished results of a previous study w i t h the same subjects. r e s e a r c h w a s partially funded by the national institutes of health, united states. r e f e r e n c e s busby, p.α., whitford, l.a., blarney, p.j., richardson, l.m. & clark, g.m. (1994). pitch perception for different modes of . stimulation using the cochlear multiple-electrode prosthesis. journal of the acoustical society of america, 95,5,2658-2669. clark, g.m., tbng, y.c. & patrick, j.f. (1990). cochlear prostheses. melbourne: churchill livingstone incorporated. dorman, m.f., soli, s., dankowski, k., smith,l.m., mccandless, g. & parkin, j. (1990). acoustic cues for consonant identification by patients who use the ineraid cochlear implant. journal of the acoustical society of america, 88,5, 2074-2079. dubno, j.r. & dorman, m.f. (1987). effects of spectral flattening on vowel identification. journal of the acoustical society of america, 82, 5, 1503-1511 eddington, d. (1980). speech discrimination in deaf subjects with cochlear implants. journal of the acoustical society of america, 68, 885-891. green, d.m. & swets, j.a. (1966). signal detection theory and psychophysics. new york: john wiley and sons incorporated. hacker, m.j. & ratcliff, r. (1979). a revised table of d' for malternative forced choice. perception and psychophysics, 26, 2, 168-170. lawson, d.t., wilson, b.s., zerbi, m. & finley, c.c. (1996). speech processors for auditory prostheses. third quarterly progress report, february through april 1996, nih contract n01-dc5-2103. nelson, d.a., van tasell, d.j., schroder, a.c. & soli, s. (1995). electrode ranking of "place pitch" and speech recognition in electrical hearing. journal of the acoustical society of america, 98, 4, 1987-1999. shannon, r.v., zeng, f-g., kamath, v., wygonski, j. & ekelid, m. (1995). speech recognition with primarily temporal cues. science, 270, 303-304. shannon, r.v., adams, d.d., ferrel, r.l., palumbo, r.l. & grandgenett, m. (1990). a computer interface for psychophysical and speech research with the nucleus cochlear implant. journal of the acoustical society of america, 87, 2, 905-907. skinner, m.w., clark, g.m., whitford, l.a., seligman, p.m., staller, s.j., shipp, d.b., shallop, j.k., everingham, c., menapace, c.m., arndt, p.l., antogenelli, t., brimacombe, j.a., pijl, s., daniels, p., george, c.r., mcdermott, h.j. & beiter, a.l. (1994). evaluation of a new spectral peak coding strategy for the nucleus 22 channel cochlear implant system. the american journal of otology, 15, supplement 2, 15-27. townshend, b. & white, r.l. (1987). reduction of electrical interaction in auditory prostheses. ieee transactions on biomedical engineering, 34, 11, 891-897. wilson, b.s., finley, c.c., lawson, d.t., wolford, r.d., eddington, d.k. & rabinowitz, w.m. (1991). new levels of speech recognition with cochlear implants. nature, 352, 236-238. zimmermann, c.e., burgess, b.j. & nadol, j.b. (1995). patterns of degeneration in the human cochlear nerve. hearing research, 90, 192-201. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 an e v a l u a t i o n of the speech perception in noise test lucille p. dickens, ba(log) (pretoria) callum m. delaney, ba (sp. and h. therapy) (witwatersrand) department of logopaedics, university of cape town, cape town abstract the effects of presentation level and signal-to-babble ratio (s/b) on spin performance were investigated for eighty normal hearing listeners. both intensity and s/b had a significant effect on scores. performance improved at the more favourable s/b regardless of presentation level, and scores were better at the lower presentation level regardless of s/b. possible clinical applications of the spin test are discussed. opsomming die uitwerking van aanbiedingsvlak en sein-tot-babbel-verhouding (s/b) op spin-toets-diskriminasietellings is ondersoek by tagtig normaalhorende luisteraars. beide aanbiedingsvlak en s/b het 'n beduidende uitwerking op diskriminasietellings. 'n hoer persentasie korrekte diskriminasie is by die meer gunstige s/b verkry, ongeag die aanbiedingsvlak, en beter diskriminasietellings is by die laer aanbiedingsvlak verkry, ongeag die s/b. kliniese toepassings van die spin-toets is bespreek. the routine procedure in clinical speech audiometry over the past few decades has been to obtain a speech reception threshold for spondaic words and an estimation of monosyllabic discrimination ability. these measures art generally obtained in quiet conditions. in spite of the seeming popularity of this approach, much criticism has been directed against it (dirks, morgan and dubno, 1982). discrimination tests using monosyllabic words have been criticized for a number of reasons, but most frequently because test stimuli and conditions do not represent typical listening environments, and because test forms are not equivalent (dirks and dubno, 1984). with this in mind kalikow, stevens and elliott (1977) developed the speech perception in noise (spin) test in order to assess the understanding of speech in noise. they recognised that in speech communication adults utilize acousticphonetic and linguistic-contextual information for perception. consequently the spin test comprises eight lists of 50 sentences each, where the predictability of the final target or key word of each sentence is controlled. in each list 25 items are designed to be primarily identified by the acoustic-phonetic information (low predictability (lp)) and the other 25 sentences include linguisticcontextual information which could aid identification (high predictability (hp)). the following are examples of each, "i had not thought about the growl" (lp) and "the watchdog gave a warning growl" (hp). since everyday speech communication commonly occurs in the presence of noise, the sentences are presented in a 12-voice background babble. the sentences and babble are recorded ori separate channels of audio tape, thus permitting variation of the signal-to-babble (s/b) ratio. in constructing the test kalikow et al. (1977) chose 250 target words from an original pool of words, with each test word presented in both an h p and an lp context in complementary lists. form equivalence for these ten lists was investigated for a group of normal listeners at 80 db spl and at a 0 db s/b. two lists were discarded, and on the basis of an analysis of variance the remaining eight were considered to be equivalent for the difference score (i.e. h p lp). while the analysis did not show similar equivalence for hp and lp scores, kalikow et al. (1977) did no feel that this was a serious problem. morgan, kamm and velde (1981) and bilger, nuetzel, rabinowitz and rzeczkowski (1984) who also examined list equivalence did not agree with the results of kalikow et al. (1977) and concluded that only seven of the original ten lists were © sasha 1986 equivalent. however, the experimental design of all three studies differed with respect to presentation level and s/b, subjects (normal hearing or hearing impaired) and statistical method of analysis. thus the results of these later studies do not necessarily contradict those of kalikow et al. (1977). the effects of variations in presentation level, s/b, age and hearing impairment on spin performance have also been investigated. a consistent finding is the expected separation between the hp and lp scores, both of which improve with improved s/b ratios (kalikow et al., 1977; hutcherson, dirks and morgan, 1979; elliott, 1979; owen, 1981). kalikow et al. (1977) found a slightly smaller difference score for an elderly group as compared to young subjects and elliott (1979) found poorer hp sentence scores for 11 and 13 year old children compared to 15 and 17 year olds, which was not apparent when the sentences were presented in quiet. this finding may lend support to owen's (1981) conclusion that differences found in difference scores are related to the audibility of the sentences rather than to the listener's use of context. from these studies no clear pattern of results or administrative protocol emerges that might make the spin test clinically useful. with these issues in mind the present study was designed to examine the performance of a group of audiologically normal subjects using a locally produced recording of the test material. the object was to collect data that might provide a basis for comparison with the spin results of hearing impaired individuals and to identify aspects of the test that might most usefully and reliably be used in a clinical context. methodology aim to investigate the performance of a group of normal hearing subjects on the spin test, and to examine the effect on performance of presentation level and signal-to-babble ratio. subjects eighty young adults aged between 18 and 29 years with normal hearing sensitivity (<15 db re: ansi 1979) at octave frequencies from 250 to 8000 hz bilaterally, served as subjects. english was the native language of all subjects, and no subject had had any previous test experience with the spin materials. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an evaluation of the speech perception in noise test 79 instrumentation the eight lists of the spin test were recorded by an englishspeaking south african male. the babble was generated by recording each of six adults (3 males and 3 females) reading the same passage from a children's story book in an anechoic chamber, mixing these six recordings and combining two repetitions of the six-voice babble to produce the final 12-voice babble. both the sentence and babble tracks were preceded by 1000 hz calibration signals. during the test sessions the lists were played on a two-channel (pioneer stereo cassette tape deck ct-f650) tape recorder. the signal and babble outputs from the tape recorder were routed to a speech audiometer (grason-stadler, gsi 10) where the intensity of each was determined separately before being mixed. the mixed output was delivered to the subject via a tdh-39 earphone mounted in a supra-aural cushion (mx 41/ar). the audiometers were calibrated according to ansi 1979 standards, and prior to each test session the vu meters of each channel were adjusted according to the 1000 hz calibration signal. all testing was conducted in dual chamber sound treated test suites. procedure lists were presented at two intensity levels (60 and 40 db η l) and two s/b ratios (0 and + 5 db). the 60 db hl 0 s/b condition was chosen to allow comparison to the kalikow et al. (1977) study. the 40 db hl level was chosen because normal to loud conversational speech falls within the 40 to 60 db hl range, and because at a 40 db sensation level (re: srt) testing would be possible for a larger percentage of hearing impaired individuals than at higher levels. the + 5 db s/b ratio was chosen because it is a more favourable condition as research (pearsons, bennett and fidell, 1976 as cited by dirks et al. 1982) has shown that this ratio is usually maintained for conversations in background noise. subjects were divided into two groups. forty subjects were tested with the odd numbered lists, and forty subjects with the four even numbered lists, thus ensuring that complementary lists were not heard by the same person. the stimuli (signal and babble) were presented to the subject's preferred ear under four listening conditions: 60 db hl with a s/b of + 5 db, 60 db hl with a s/b of 0 db;, 40 db hl with a s/b of + 5 db, and 40 db hl with a s/b of 0 db: the order of presentation remained constant, but the order of presenting the lists was varied so that each list was presented under each listening condition the same number of times. subjects were instructed to write down the last word of each sentence. analysis o f results a strict scoring protocol was adopted. (singular/plural conversions were not acceptable). total, hp, lp and difference scores were converted into percentages. summary statistics (means and standard deviations) were used to describe central tendencies for each of the listening conditions. data were subjected to a two factor analysis of variance with repeated measures of both factors (treatments-by-treatments-by-subjects aov) to assess the effects of presentation level and s/b ratio. results and discussion table 1 summarizes the mean scores and standard deviations found for all the lists under the different listening conditions. table 1: means and standard deviations of scores obtained under four listening conditions. listening conditions 60 db 40 db total score x 80,34 52,53 85,48 59,73 (hp+lp) sd 8,89 11,77 6,42 13,04 hp score x 96,15 73,00 98,10 78,50 sd 5,30 13,07 3,49 13,94 lp score x 64,30 32,05 72,20 40,95 sd 14,66 13,51 11,04 14,86 difference score x 31,90 40,95 25,90 37,50 (hp-lp) sd 13,44 12,36 10,83 11.99 the results obtained at 60 db 0 s/b in the present study are slightly lower than those reported by kalikow et al. (1977), who reported the following mean values: hp = 87,8 lp = 40,3, and difference = 47,4. means for the other conditions have not been reported in the literature. there is a difference for scores obtained at the two s/b ratios for each intensity as well as at the two presentation levels for each s/b ratio. four 2-way analyses of variance with repeated measures on both factors were performed to determine whether scores (total, hp, lp and difference) obtained under four listening conditions (60 db, + 5 s/b, 60 db, 0 s/b, 40 db, + 5 s/b and 40 db, 0 s/b) differed significantly. the results of these anova's showed no significant interaction between intensity level and s/b ratio. both main effects (intensity level and s/b ratio) were significant (p<0.01), mean scores being better at the more favourable s/b ratio regardless of s/b ratio. the f values obtained are summarised in table 2. (in each case the degrees of freedom were 1,79.) table 2: f-values obtained in four anova's to test the difference between four scores (total hp, lp and difference) obtained under four listening conditions. total hp lp difference source intensity 74,69* 14,43* 78,69* 13,40* s/b 888,17* 435,08* 689,27* 54,07* intensity χ χ 0,94 2,86 0,13 1,06 s/b * ρ 0 . 0 1 the finding that all scores improve at the more favourable s/b ratio regardless of presentation level was to be expected and confirms results from previous studies (hutcherson et al., 1979; owen, 1981). an unexpected finding was that scores were better at the lower presentation level for both s/b conditions. this finding is contradictory to that of hutcherson et al. (1979) who found little difference in scores at 50 db and 80 db spl, and a definite improvement from 30 to 50 db spl. since the order of testing under the four listening conditions was held constant it may be postulated that a learning effect produced this result. however, in such a case less of a difference would be expected between the 60/40 db 0 s/b conditions than the +5 s/b conditions, but examination of the results indicates that the difference is of equal magnitude. a similar intensity effect appears to be present in data reported for thee normal hearing subjects for words from nu # 6 lists presented in spin background babble (dirks et al., 1982). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 l.p. dickens and c.m. delaney taken together these results may suggest that there is an optimal presentation level for discrimination of speech in noise, and that an increase or decrease in this level will result in a deterioration of performance. this level may correspond to that of conversational speech (40 db hl). the eftect should be more thoroughly explored — for both the normal hearing population and those with sensorineural hearing losses. it would be interesting to determine how, for example, individuals with cochlear losses and concomitant intolerance for loud sounds would perform. the immediate clinical implication of these results is that the spin test should not be administered under listening conditions for which normative data is unavailable. regardless of the measure used, both intensity and s/b affect performance. consequently no generalizations about scores can be made. the present findings provide a means for comparing the performance of hearing impaired individuals with that of normal listeners. for such a purpose it is suggested that the 40 db +5 s/b protocol is adopted. at the 40 db sl re srt presentation level testing is likely to be possible for the majority of hearing impaired listeners, and the +5 db s/b provides the most well defined normal performance. the good hp scores and the relatively high lp scores would allow measurement of the poorer performance by the hearing-impaired individual over a wider range than would be possible under the other three conditions. the hp and lp scores provide two sources of information. they provide an indication of performance that can be compared with normal performance. in addition the relationship between the hp and lp scores for an individual provide an indication of the extent to which he is taking advantage of sentence context, and this has important therapeutic implications. however, this relationship is only meaningful in the context of the normal hp andlp scores. considering that noise has a differential effect on individuals even with similar audiometric configurations and degree of loss (plomp and mimpen, 1979), determination of performance functions for various s/b ratios and intensity levels would give the best estimate of ability to understand speech at suprathreshold levels. however, in its present form the spin test would not be a cost effective or practical method for this purpose, being too time-consuming and fatiguing. in conclusion it is suggested that the spin test be administered at the 40 db + 5 s/b level in order to obtain comparative and rehabilitative information. any diagnostic application of the spin test among hearing-impaired individuals requires further research. references bilger, r.c., nuetzel, j.m., rabinowitz, w.m., and rzeczkowski, c. standardization of a test of speech perception in noise. j. speech hear. res., 27, 32-48. 1984. dirks, d.d., morgan, d.e., and dubno, j.r. a procedure for quantifying the effects of noise on speech recognition. j. speech hear. disord., 47, 114-123, 1982. dirks, d.d., and dubno, j.r. speech audiometry. in j. jerger (ed.) hearing disorders in adults: current trends, san diego: college-hill press, 1984. elliott, l.l. performance of children aged 9 to 17 years on a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence material with controlled word predictability. j. acous. soc., am., 66, 651-653, 1979. hutcherson, r.w., dirks, d.d.,and morgan, d.e. evaluation of the speech perception in noise (spin) test. otolaryngol. head neck surg., 87, 239-245, 1979. kalikow, d.n., stevens, k.n., and elliott, l.l. development of a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence materials with controlled word predictability. j. acoust. soc., am., 61, 1337-1351, 1977. morgan, d.e., kamm, c.a., and velde, t.m. form equivalence of the speech perception in noise (spin) test. j.acoust.soc., am., 69, 1791-1798, 1981. owen, j.h. influence of acoustical and linguistic factors on the spin test difference score. j. acoust. soc. am., 70,678-682, 1982. plomp, r., and mimpen, a.r. speech reception threshold for sentences as a function of age and noise level. j. acoust. soc. am., 66, 1333-1342, 1979. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 81 information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, or critically evaluative theoretical, or therapeutic issues dealing with disorders of speech, voice, hearing or language, or on aspects of the processes underlying these. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. all contributions are reviewed by at least two consultants who are not provided with author identification. form of manuscript. authors should submit four neatly typewritten manuscripts in triple spacing with wide margins which should not exceed much more than 25 pages. each page should be numbered. the first page of two copies should contain the title of the article, name of author/s, highest degree and address or institutional affiliation. the first page of the remaining two copies should contain only the title of the article. the second page of all copies should contain only an abstract (100 words) which should be provided in both english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors. all paragraphs should start at the left margin and not be indented. major headings, where applicable, should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements and references. tables and figures should be prepared on separate sheets (one per table/figure). figures, graphs and line drawings must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper. lettering appearing on these should be uniform and professionally done, bearing in mind that such lettering should be legible after a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should not be included in the illustration but should appear below it. the titles of tables and figures should be concise but explanatory. the title of tables appears above, and of figures below. tables and figures should be numbered in order of appearance (with arabic numerals). the amount of tabular and illustrative material allowed will be at the discretion of the editor (usually not more than 6). references. references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and date, e.g. van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, et al. after the first author will suffice. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list. references should be listed alphabetically in triple-spacing at the end of the article. for acceptable abbreviations of names of journals, consult the fourth issue (october) of dsh abstracts or the world list of scientific periodicals. the number of references used should not exceed much more than 20. note the following examples: locke, j.l. clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed^handbook of clinical audiology, 3rd ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. proofs. galley proofs will be sent to the author wherever possible. corrections other than typographical errors will be charged to the author. reprints. 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. all manuscripts and correspondence should be addressed to: the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech and hearing association, p.o. box 31782, braamfontein 2017, south africa. inligting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer verslae en artikels oor navorsing, of krities evaluerende artikels oor die teoretiese of terapeutiese aspekte van spraak-, stem-, gehoorof taalafwykings, of oor aspekte van die prosesse onderliggend aan hierdie afwykings. ι die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings sal nie materiaal aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. alle bydraes word deur minstens twee konsultante nagegaan wat nie ingelig is oor die identiteit van iiie skrywer nie. formaat van die manuskrip. skrywers moet vier netjies getikte manuskripte in 3-spasiering en met bree kantlyn indien, en dit moet nie veel langer as 25 bladsye wees nie. elke bladsy moet genommer wees. | op die eerste bladsy van 2 afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, die naam van die skrywer/s, die hoogste graad behaal en die adres of naam van hulle betrokke instansie verskyn. op die eerste bladsy van die oorblywende twee afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskyn. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans be vat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. alle paragrawe moet teenaan die linkerkantlyn begin word en moet nie ingekeep word nie. hoofopskrifte moet, waar dit van toepassing is, in die volgende volgorde wees: metode, resultate, bespreking, gevolgtrekking, erkennings en verwysings. tabelle en figure moet op afsonderlike bladsye verskyn (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie). figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge moet oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte gedoen word. letterwerk wat hierop verskyn moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50%-verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by die illustrasie moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verkladie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 rings of omskry wings moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maar daaronder verskyn. die byskrifte van tabelle moet bo-aan verskyn en die van figure onderaan. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word (met arabiese syfers). die hoeveelheid materiaal in die vorm van tabelle en illustrasies wat toegelaat word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). verwysings. verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, bv. van riper (1971). waar daar meer as twee skrywers is, sal et al. na die eerste skrywer voldoende wees. die name van alle skrywers moet in die verwysingslys verskyn. verwysings moet alfabeties in 3-spasiering aan die einde van die artikel gerangskik, word. vir die aanvaarde afkortings van tydskrifte se titels, raadpleeg die vierde uitgawe (oktober) van dsh abstracts of the world list of scientific periodicals. die getal verwysings wat gebruik is, moet nie veel meer as 20 wees nie. let op die volgende voorbeelde: locke, j.l. clinical phonology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord, 48 339-341 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed^handbook of clinical audiology, 3de ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice hall, 1971. proewe. galeiproewe sal waar moontlik aan die skrywer gestuur word. die onkoste van veranderings, behalwe tipografiese foute, sal deur die skrywer self gedra moet word. herdrukke. 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis verskaf word. alle manuskripte en korrespondensie moet gerig word aan: die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings. die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraaken gehoorheelkunde, posbus 31782, braamfontein 2017, suid-afrka. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 59 speech discrimination in the elderly l.p. dickens, ba(log) (pretoria) department of logopaedics, university of cape town. abstract the performance of 39 elderly subjects on three speech discrimination tasks, viz, cid yj-22 word lists, the spin test and compressed spin sentence lists, was investigated. the effects of age and audiometric configuration on discrimination ability were examined, and age was found to significantly differentiate between subjects while audiometric configuration did not prove to be significantly variable. with the exception of the hp sentences for the spin and compressed speech tests, scores for all other measures differ significantly and thus would seem to assess different aspects of discrimination. the implications of these results are discussed. opsomming die prestasie van 39 bejaarde proefpersone op drie spraakdiskriminasietoetse, naamlik cid w-22 woordlyste, die spin-toets en saamgeperste spin-sinlyste, is ondersoek. die invloed van ouderom en oudiometriese konfigurasie op diskriminasievermoe is bestudeer en daar is gevind dat ouderdom beduidend differensieer tussen proefpersone, terwyl oudiometriese konfigurasie nie 'n beduidende variant blyk te wees nie. met die uitsondering van die hp-sinne van die spin en saamgeperste spraaktoetse, het resultate vir alle ander metings beduidend verskil en dit wil dus voorkom asof die toetse verskillende aspekte van diskriminasie ondersoek. die implikasies van hierdie resultate word bespreek. introduction the fact that hearing sensitivity deteriorates with age has been known for many years. the characteristic high-frequency nature of the loss was identified by zwaardemaker as early as 1899 (cited by arnst, 1985). the initial studies were mainly concerned with documenting the amount of hearing loss occurring at various frequencies (bunch, 1929, 1931 cited by willeford, 1978), but following the reports by gaeth (1948, cited by willeford, 1978) of discrimination problems experienced by the elderly, which he called ' 'phonemic regression", interest has been focused on speech discrimination ability. the descriptive term used to denote this deterioration of auditory function that accompanies the aging process is presbycusis (krnst, 1985, garstecki, 1981). the more recent descriptions (miller, 1983; marshall, 1981; osterhammel and osterhammel, 1979; and willeford 1978) of it being a slowly progressive, bilaterally symmetrical, mildly sloping sensorineural loss. with speech discrimination skills that are poorer than would be expected for the amount of hearing loss concur with the earlier ones. anatomical changes due to aging have been found throughout the auditory system, and these have been extensively and unequivocally documented, e.g., valenstein, 1981; nadol 1981; schuknecht, 1974, and consequently will not be dealt with in this paper. however, there is less conformity amongst the reports concerning the effect of age on auditory function, and more especially speech discrimination ability. many studies have compared word discrimination with sensitivity loss, and have attributed the disproportionate problems with speech perception to central auditory nervous system (cans) degeneration (e.g., bergman, 1971; pestalozza and shore, 1955). a number of practical issues have complicated these investigations; thus our knowledge about the normal deterioration of hearing with age is limited (miller, 1983) and further evaluation of the behavioural effects of presbycusis are needed (marshall, 1981; hayes, 1984). in investigating speech discrimination the majority of studies only measured performance at one sensation level, usually 30-40 db sl re srt (marshall, 1981). however, it has been shown that maximum discrimination in the elderly may only occur at much higher levels (kasden, 1970), thus it may be reasonable to expect that performance may have been underestimated. generally, the early studies were concerned with monosyllabic word discrimination in ideal conditions, yet plomp and mimpen (1979) have established that the problems experienced by the elderly primarily manifest themselves in noisy environments. while some investigators examined the effects of noise on word and sentence identification (e.g., jerger and hayes, 1977; arnst, 1985) results are conflicting and the stimuli used were not representative of everyday listening conditions. in order to define the problems experienced by the elderly, more realistic speech discrimination measures need to be used (marshall, 1981). a measure which seems to be more appropriate for this task is the spin test. kalikow, stevens and elliott (1977) developed this test taking into account the fact that normal adults utilise both acoustic-phonetic and linguistic-contextual cues for speech recognition, and that everyday communication occurs in a background of noise. the test examines discrimination as a function of both context and competing noise, and research has established that, as would be expected, the performance of normal hearing young adults improves when contextual cues are present and the noise level is decreased (kalikow et al. 1977; dickens & delaney, 1986). however, besides a brief preliminary report on the performance of ten elderly subjects whose scores were slightly die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 © sasha 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 l.p. dickens depressed relative to those of younger listeners (kalikow et al. 1977), no research (to the writer's knowledge) has concerned itself with investigating this aspect in the elderly population. central auditory processing can be seen as a kind of speech discrimination (willeford, 1978), and research has suggested that reduced cans function becomes evident when the speech is degraded in some manner. speech can be degraded in many ways, e.g., by filtering, by time compression, by the introduction of competing messages, etc. compressed speech provides a quick and simple means of assessing cans function, and was first used by calearo and lazzaroni in 1957. these authors documented the problems that the elderly, in comparison to young listeners, experience with fast speech. since this report much interest has been shown in this area, but subsequent studies have not all been in agreement. while konkle, beasley and bess (1977) for example, confirmed the findings of calearo and lazzaroni, others such as miller (1983) and luterman, welsh and melrose (1966) found no difference between young and elderly listeners, and schmitt (1983) and schmitt and mccroskey (1981) reported improved performance for compressed speech in a group of the elderly. a variety of procedures and materials were used in these studies. according to beasley and maki (1976) one needs to consider whether data from a specific study reflects upon intelligibility or comprehension. monosyllabic stimuli would provide information pertaining to intelligibility, but may be inadequate for assessing the linguistic integrity of the central processing system, especially in cases where there is concomitant peripheral involvement (beasley, bratt and rintelmann, 1980). since this is likely to be the situation with the elderly, it would seem appropriate to use sentential stimuli for this purpose, and material such as the spin lists appear to be ideal, combining as they do monosyllabic and sentential features. finally, the effects of hearings loss, as opposed to age per se, do not appear to have been examined in much detail. marshall (1981) suggests that peripheral problems other than sensitivity loss may underlie those problems experienced by the elderly. dubno and dirks (1982, 1984) have reported a strong relationship between audiogram shape and speech discrimination, and this needs to be investigated in relation to the elderly. it is evident that the effects of aging on speech discrimination are not clearly understood and that the previous use of the term "phonemic regression" to explain the poor speech discrimination observed in some elderly individuals needs to be reassessed. methodology aims to investigate the performance of a group of elderly subjects on "three speech discrimination tasks viz. cid w-22 word lists, the spin test, and time-compressed speech, and to examine the differential effect on performance of age as opposed to audiogram configuration. subjects subjects (ss) were selected from five old age homes located in the cape town area. at each of these volunteers were requested from a group of residents that had been preselected by the matron or sister-in-charge. potential ss were required to be over 60 years of age, english speaking, show no signs of senile dementia, and be normally active, i.e., not confined to a wheelchair or bed. while sex was not a criterion, it was hoped that a reasonable proportion of males to females would be obtained. however, there were very few male residents, and only a small percentage of these conformed to all the criteria and were willing to act as ss. 39 ss (78 ears) participated in the study, 36 females and 3 males. the age range was 60 to 87 years, with a mean age of 74.2. no ss were excluded from the study on the basis of previous history of hearing problems or noise exposure, because the aim was to examine speech discrimination abilities in the aged population as it presents itself, as was suggested by miller (1983). furthermore it has been suggested that it is unrealistic to expect accurate recall of any confounding factors over a 60 year life-span or longer (osterhammel and osterhammel, 1979; hayes, 1984). materials and instrumentation all speech materials were prerecorded by the same englishspeaking south african male. the cid w-22 lists were recorded in quiet, while the spin lists were presented in a 12-voice background-babble, with sentences and babble recorded on separate channels allowing for variations of the signal-to-babble (s/n) ratio. lists 1 and 3 were used in the present study. the compressed speech was obtained by processing the recorded spin lists (6, 7 and 8) without the background babble through a lexicon varispeech model ii. the speed factor dial was set at 2, which corresponds to a 100% compression rate. the procedure for selecting this speed factor dial setting and the collection of normative data has been described elsewhere (dickens, 1987). list 7 was used for practice. spin lists were used for compressed tasks because, as previously discussed, they combine monosyllabic and sentential ίεβΐμγββ, and in addition would allow for direct comparison of the effects of noise and compression on discrimination. all audiometric testing was conducted on a madsen micro-5 digital audiometer, with tdh-39 earphones mounted in mx/ar supra-aural aircushions, to which a two-channel (pioneer stereo tape deck — ct-f650) tape recorder was connected. the audiometer was calibrated according to ansi 1979 standards, and prior to each test session the vu-meters of each channel were adjusted according to the 1000 hz calibration signal. testing was conducted in a dual chamber sound-treated test suite. procedure prior to assessing performance on the speech discrimination tasks, pure tone (air and bone conduction) and speech thresholds were obtained for each subject. these were necessary in order to group ss according to audiogram configuration, and to determine presentation levels for the speech tasks. all speech measures were obtained separately for each ear under earphones. cid w-22 lists were presented at 20̂ 40 and 60 db sl (re srt) unless the 60 db level exceeded the threshold of discomfort, in which case a lower sl was used when necessary. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech discrimination in the elderly spin lists were presented at 40 db sl (re srt) at a s/n ratio of +5 db, as was suggested by dickens and delaney (1986). compressed speech (cspin) lists were also presented at 40 db sl (re srt), to allow for comparison between the various measures. 61 do not differ much for mean age or pta, and that the standard deviations for these measures are similar for all three groups. table 1: mean age and pta values for ss grouped according to age ss were instructed to respond verbally. analysis of results all scores were converted into percentages. the total high predictability (hp) and low predictability (lp) scores for both the spin and cspin tests and the maximum discrimination scores (regardless of sl) were used in the analysis. summary statistics were calculated to describe the central tendencies. data were subjected to two factor analyses of variance (anova) with repeated measures on one factor (treatments-by-groups) to examine the difference between scores obtained for grouping according to age as opposed to audiogram configuration. multivariate analyses and the scheffe test were used to determine the specific comparison which accounted for the significant f-ratios within the interactions. grouping was as follows: by age in decade: group i — 60 to 69 years group ii — 70 to 79 years group iii — 80 years and older by audiometric configuration: the system described by dubno and dirks (1982) was used. group a group β / group c — flat; < or = 20 db difference in threshold from 250 to 4000 hz. — gradually sloping; 25 to 40 db difference in threshold from 250 to 4000 hz, and < 30 db difference in! threshold between adjacent octave frequencies. — steeply sloping; > 40 db difference in threshold from 250 to 4000 hz, or > 30 db difference between adjacent octave frequencies. results and discussion pta age group i x 25.77 64.91 sd 15.33 3.93 group ii x 33.44 74.72 sd 11.81 3.04 group iii x 39.90 83.30 sd 17.66 2.20 table 2: mean age and pta values for ss grouped according to audiogram configuration age pta group a x 74.40 33.19 sd 8.10 13.26 group β x 73.70 30.94 sd 6.65 13.86 group c x 74,75 32.00 sd 7.98 13.61 table i illustrates the mean pure tone average (pta) for each of the three age groups. as can be seen, there is a slight increase in pta with an increase in age. the mean age and pta for ss grouped according to audiometric configuration can be seen in table 2. it is interesting to note that the groups table 3: means and standard deviations of scores obtained on the speech measures the means and standard deviations obtained for the speech measures can be seen in table 3. the results of the anovas to examine the difference between the speech scores for the ss grouped according to age and audiogram configuration are summarised in table 4 and 5. these show that when ss were grouped according to age there was a significant variance for both main effects, i.e., age and speech score, while for ss grouped according to audiogram configuration there was a significant difference for speechscore, but not for audiogram configuration. from these results, it can be concluded that ss scored differently on the different speech tasks, and that age rather than audiogram configuration is the critical factor which differentiates between ss. the lack of differentiation between the audiometric groups could possibly be explained by the fact that, for some of the speech tasks, sentence stimuli were used, i.e., the spin and cspin hp items, and dubno and dirks (1982) report that the relationship between discrimination and audiometric configuration is weakened when sentences are used as stimuli. however, this does not explain the lack of variance between the ss for the other speech measures, viz. the cid w-22 maximum and the lp items of the spin and cspin tests. a more likely explanation may be that the difference between the groups is qualitative rather than quantitative. another confounding variable may be related to sample size. group cid w-22 spin spin spin cspin cspin cspin group max hp , lp total hp lp total group i x 91.46 88.38 45.90 67.14 86.29 69.71 78.00 (60-69) sd 13.14 8.76 12.50 8.75 20.30 23.41 20.00 group ii x 88.89 79.54 37.83 58.69 80.12 58.91 70.06 (70-79) sd 9.47 14.32 20.20 14.91 14.91 17.67 12.61 group iii x 72.84 62.35 18.82 40.59 60.00 37.18 48.59 (80 + ) sd 19.67 25.65 13.91 18.79 27.13 24.36 24.35 all subjects x 85:66 78.08 35.73 56.90 77.13 56.90 67.27 all subjects sd 15.37 18.83 19.46 17.31 22.10 24.10 21.10 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 l.p. dickens table 4: a n o v a summary table showing interaction between age (a) and speech score (s) cid w-22 χ spin η χ cspin η source df f ρ cid w-22 χ spin η χ cspin η a 2 12.30 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin η χ cspin η s 2 26.28 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin η χ cspin η sxa 4 1.81 .1299 cid w-22 χ spin l χ cspin l a 2 14.84 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin l χ cspin l s 2 359.38 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin l χ cspin l sxa 4 2.43 .0509 cid w-22 χ spin τ χ cspin τ a 2 15.65 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin τ χ cspin τ s 2 235.15 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin τ χ cspin τ sxa 4 3.19 .0154 table 5: a n o v a summary table showing interaction between audiogram configuration (g) and speech score (r). cid w-22 χ spin η χ cspin η source df f ρ cid w-22 χ spin η χ cspin η g 2 1.48 .2346 cid w-22 χ spin η χ cspin η r 2 22.76 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin η χ cspin η rxg 4 1.98 .1004 cid w-22 χ spin l χ cspin l g 2 1.95 .1502 cid w-22 χ spin l χ cspin l r 2 253.86 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin l χ cspin l rxg 4 1.46 .2188 cid w-22 χ spin τ χ cspin τ g 2 1.82 .1703 cid w-22 χ spin τ χ cspin τ r 2 170.26 .0000 cid w-22 χ spin τ χ cspin τ rxg 4 2.29 .0632 the variance in age was further investigated by means of multi-variate analysis and scheffe post hoc comparisons. the scheffe test results are summarised in table 6. these indicate that the scores obtained by group iii differed significantly from those obtained by the other two groups. this difference was apparent for all the speech measures. while the mean values for the speech tasks seen in table 3 show a tendency for performance to deteriorate with increasing age for all the tests, the difference between groups i and ii was not found to be statistically significant. this performance difference could reflect a loss effect, since mean pta scores also reflect an increase with corresponding increase in age. however, t-test computations (summarised in table 7) show that the pta values for group i differ significantly from groups ii and iii, with no significant difference between the latter two groups. this is in contrast to the speech score differences, where groups i and ii were not statistically differentiated. the data therefore suggests a complex interaction between age and degree of loss. this is in agreement with a study by bess and townsend (1977) which showed that discrimination ability is dependent on both age and degree of loss, i.e., discrimination as a function of age only decreased for individuals whose pta scores exceeded 49 db, while there was no age effect for ss with pta scores below 49 db. the mean pta values for the three groups in the present study are all below 49 db, and a general trend for an agerelated reduction in discrimination was evident. the fact that bess and townsend (1977) did not see much of an age effect for ss with smaller pta scores could be due to their age grouping, i.e., by twenty year spans rather than by decade. table 6: summary of scheffe values showing significant age group variation for specific speech tasks. speech score αι—a2 a i a 3 a2—a3 a j = 60-69 max. cid w-22 1.2 4.215 + + + 3.487 + + a2 = 70-79 spin η 1.6741 4.8738+++ 3.7609** a3 = 80 + cspin η 1.1064 4.0337+" 3.3736++ spin l 1.7360 4.8347+++ 3.6602" cspin l 1.8283 4.7118+++ 3.4397" spin τ 1.952 5.675 + + + 4.448 cspin τ 1.5613 4.948 + + + 3.9365+++ f crit 2,68: 2.501 : ρ <0.05 + 3.138 : p<0.01 + + 3.908 : ρ <0.001 + + + table 7: summary of t-test values showing significant pta difference comparison t-value groups i χ ii 2.085 + groups ii x iii 1.822 groups i x i i i 2.776 + + + ρ 0.05 + + ρ 0.01 the present results indicate that a significant age x degree', of loss interaction occurs when the loss exceeds 33 db (meanj pta value for group ii), and that the trend for scores to1 deteriorate with increasing age is also apparent for smaller' pta values. , the distribution of the cid w-22 maximum scores for each group and the entire sample can be seen in figure 1. this shows that the majority of ss (56%) had discrimination scores which were better than 91%, which can be seen to refute the statement that disproportionately poor speech discrimination scores are a general feature of age-related hearing loss. this supports miller's (1981, cited by miller, 1983) finding, although the present proportion is slightly lower than miller's. the reason for this may be twofold; miller only used 70 year old ss (reassessed at 75), and their average loss was less severe. in addition to the quantitative difference betweengroups iii, and i and ii, figure 1 also illustrates the qualitative differences (in terms of general distribution). groups i and ii folthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech discrimination in the elderly 63 examination of the spin scores shows that there is the expected hp and lp separation, and that the scores are reduced in relation to those reported for normal hearing young ss (dickens and delaney, 1986). while both hp and lp scores are reduced, the reduction is much larger for the lp items, and this becomes more pronounced with increasing age. this was also seen for the compressed speech. scores collected from 30 normal hearing ss were: total equals 90.00 (3.58), hp = 97.09 (3.14), and lp = 82.91 (6.95) (dickens, 1987). an interesting finding is that the standard deviations for all groups tend to be larger for compressed speech. this would seem to support miller's contention that in a "non-clinical" group of elderly, there are those individuals with well-preserved central auditory function as well as those with presumable central auditory problems. the results of the scheffe computations to investigate significant speech score variation within the different age groups can be seen in table 8. for group i there was no significant difference between scores obtained for the cid w-22, the spin hp and the cspin hp procedures, and for both groups ii and iii there was no difference between the spin hp and cspin hp scores. for all the other measures the speech score variation was found to be significant within all three age groups, indicating that the tests assess different aspects of auditory discrimination. the lack of significant difference for the hp measures for all ss, regardless of age or audiometric configuration, is interestable 8: summary table of scheffe values showing significant speech score variation within age groups. groups sj — s2 s, s , s2 — s3 si = cis w-22 max. a, (60-69) 1.6829 2.4219 .0738 52 = spin η 53 = cspin η a2 (70-79) 3.701 + + 3.9755+++ .2703 / / a3 (79 +) 4.4917 +~ 5.2393*" .7476 groups si — s2 s i s , s2 — s3 s, = cis w-22 max. a! (60 69) 13.643 6.7667 +++ 6.8764+++ 52 = spin l 1 53 = cspin l j a2 (70 79) 18.647 10.925 7.7215+++ a, (79 +) 14.9788+" 10.2084"+ 4.7703+++ groups si — s2 s i s , s2 — s3 s, cis w-22 max. a, (60 69) 10.198 5.938 + + + 4.2596+++ 52 = spin τ 53 = cspin τ a2 • (70 79) 14.721 * ++ 9.3587"* 5.364 a3 (79 +) 13.368 9.839 + + + 2.823 + f crit 2,68: 2.501 : ρ<0.05 + 3.138 : p<0.01 + + 3.908 : p<0.001 + + + 100% i [ all s u b j e c t s θ g r o u p i [jj[j] g r o u p ii q g r o u p iii figure 1. cid w-22 maximum discrimination scores low a similar trend (which is the trend shown by the entire sample), i.e., the majority of ss show good discrimination, with only 9% and 14% respectively, obtaining scores below 76%. the pattern for group iii differs, showing a flatter distribution curve with the majority (37%) having scores below 76%. this further illustrates the complex interactive effects of age and degree of loss on discrimination. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 l.p. dickens ting. introduction of noise and compression did not have a differential effect on hp performance, while for the lp sentences the two conditions were differentiated. thus, the important feature seems to be context, which apparently helps to overcome the distortion, regardless of the nature of the distortion. this has important implications for rehabilitation with the elderly hearing impaired client, in that more emphasis should be placed on teaching him to utilise contextual information in order to overcome problems which he experiences in the less than ideal type of communicative situation. it would also be interesting to investigate the performance of different groups of ss, including those with cans disorders, to determine whether any demonstrate an hp discrepancy for the spin and cspin tests, and thus whether this could be used diagnostically. for all ss the scores obtained for the cid w-22 lists were the highest, with cspin and spin following in that order. very often the elderly specifically complain of problems in understanding speech in noise, yet this aspect is not routinely investigated. the present results indicate that the ss perform differently in noise and quiet, supporting the research by plomp and mimpen (1979), which showed that ss with the same hearing loss for speech in quiet may differ in their performance in noise, and that this could not really be predicted from the performance in quiet. the fact that the scores for the speech measures in quiet and noise differ significantly suggests that different aspects of auditory function are being measured. a study by festen and plomp (1983) provides support for this. they investigated the relationship between various auditory functions in hearing impaired individuals, and concluded that hearing loss for speech in quiet is determined by audiometric loss, while hearing loss for speech in noise is governed by frequency resolution abilities. if different aspects of auditory function are being measured, as this and previous research suggests, it is necessary to include, in the routine audiologic battery, a test which evaluates discrimination in noise. all the ss performed more poorly in the spin than on the cspin test. the reduction in performance on both the cspin and the spin tests could be seen as evidence of central processing problems (i.e., compressed speech is generally considered to be sensitive to disorders in the central auditory nervous system, and all ss performed more poorly on the spin). it could be argued that the noise element in the spin test reduces the redundancy of the speech signal to a greater degree than the compression in the cspin test and, that it it is therefore tapping more subtle cans disorders. however, the more likely explanation is that reduced performance on the spin is the result of peripheral processing problems, and that the reduction in performance on the cspin may also be due to the peripheral involvement rather than central problems. the study of plomp and mimpen (1979) which investigated srt for sentences as function of age and noise appears to support this, i.e., they found that for ss up until about 90 years of age, the critical variable in hearing loss for speech is deterioration in auditory processing rather than in central processing. in conclusion, it is evident that, except for the hp sections of the spin and cspin tests, all other measures appear to be assessing different aspects of auditory discrimination, and that age rather than audiometric configuration is the distinguishing factor. as a group the elderly perform fairly well on conventional speech discrimination tasks (i.e., cid w-22 lists in quiet) but this does not reflect the problem that they may have in different situations. since the most common complaint of the elderly patient in a clinic is the difficulty that he experiences with speech in noisy situations it is recommended that a speech in noise test be administered routinely. it may be advisable to administer both the spin and cspin tests, since this may provide potentially important diagnostic and rehabilitative information concerning the differential effect of distortion. at this stage it is not clear whether the problems that the elderly experience with compressed speech and especially with speech in noise is a reflection of cans disorder or breakdown in peripheral auditory processing. before any final comment can be made concerning the question of central processing problems in the elderly, further research needs to be conducted, firstly, to determine the sensitivity of the spin and cspin in detecting cans disorders in patients with documented lesions and, secondly, to establish the influence of peripheral sensitivity loss on young listeners' performance on these tests. references arnst, d.j. presbycusis. in j. katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology, 3rd edition baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. beasley, d.s. & maki, j.e. time and frequency altered speech. in n.j. lass (ed.) contemporary issues in experimental phonetics. new york: academic press, 1976. beasley d.s., bratt, g. & rintelmann, w. intelligibility of timecompressed sentential stimuli./. speech hear. res., 23, 722739, 1980. bergman, m. hearing and aging, audiology 10, 164-171, 1971. bergman, m. central auditory disorders. in r. hinchcliffe (ed.) hearing and balance in the elderly. new york: churchill livingstone, 1983. bess, f.h. & townsend, t.h. word discrimination for listeners with flat sensorineural hearing losses. j. speech hear. disord., 42, 232-237, 1977. calearo, c. & lazzaroni, a. speech intelligibility in relation to the speed of the message, laryngoscope, 67, 410-419, 1957. dickens, l.p. & delaney, c.m. an evaluation of the speech percep-j tion in noise test. s.a. j. comm. dis., 33, 78-80, 1986. j dickens, l.p. audiological correlates of aging. unpublishedl master's dissertation, university of the witwatersrand, 1987. 1 dubno, j. & dirks, d. evaluation of hearing impaired listeners us-1 ing a nonsense-syllable test. i. test reliability./. speech j hear. res., 25, 135-141, 1982. dirks, d.d. & dubno, j.r. speech audiometry. in j. jerger (ed. j hearing disorders in adults: current trends. san diego: college-hill press, 1984. 1 festen, j.m. & plomp, r. relations between auditory functions in impaired hearing./. acous. soc. am., till, 652-662, 1983 garstecki, d.c. aural rehabilitation for the aging adult. in d. beasley & g. davis (eds.) aging: communication processes and disorders. new york: grune & stratton, 1981. hayes, d. hearing problems in aging. in j. jerger (ed.) hearing, disorders in adults: current trends. san diego: college-hill press, 1984. jerger, j. & hayes, d. diagnostic speech audiometry. arch. otolaryngol, 102, 216-222, 1977. / kalikow, d.n., stevens, k.n. & elliot, l.l. development^ a test of speech intelligibility in noise using sentence materials with controlled word predictability./. acousfsoc. am., 61,5 1337-1351, 1977 ^ kasden, s.d. speech discrimination in two'age groups matched for hearing loss./. aud. res., 10, 210-212, 1970. konkle, d.f., beasley, d.s. & bess, f.h. intelligibility of timealtered speech in relation to chronological aging. j. speech hear. res., 20, 108-115, 1977. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34,1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech discrimination in the elderly 65 luterman, d.m. welsh, c.l. & melrose, j. responses of aged males to time-altered speech stimuli./. speech. res., 9, 226230, 1966. marshall, l. auditory processing in aging listeners./. speech hear. disord, 46, 226-240, 1981. m011er, m.b. changes in hearing measures with increasing age. in r. hinchcliffe (ed.) hearing and balance in the elderly. new york: churchill livingstone, 1983. nadol. j.b. the aging peripheral hearing mechanism. in d.s. beasley & g.a. davis (eds.] aging: communication processes and disorders. new york: grune & stratton, 1981. osterhammel, d. & osterhammel, p. high frequency and audiometry: age and sex variation. scand. audiol. 8, 73-81, 1979. pestalozza, g. & shore, i. clinical evaluation of presbycusis on the basis of different tests of auditory function. laryngoscope, 65, 1136-1163, 1955. plomp, r. & mimpen, a.r. speech-reception threshold for sentences as a function of age and noise level./. acous. soc. am., 66, 1333-1343, 1979. schmitt, j.f. & mccroskey, r.l. sentence comprehension in elderly listeners: the factor of rate./. gerontology, 36, 441-445, 1981. schmitt, j.f. the effects of time compression and time expansion on passage comprehension by the elderly. /. speech hear. res., 26,373-377, 1983. schuknecht, h.f. pathology of the ear. campbride mass: harvard university press, 1974. valenstein, e. age-related changes in the human central nervous system. in d.s. beasley & g.a. davis (eds.) aging: communication processes and disorders. new york: grune & stratton, 1981. willeford, j.a. the geriatric patient. in d. rose (ed.) audiological assessment, 2nd ed., new yersey: prentice-hall, 1978. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c the stanoaflo of silence industrial acoustics company '™ ketton road, wadeville ext. 5 (011) 902-4492/3 14337, wadeville 1422 we set the standards for your audiologic equipment throughout south africa industrial acoustics professional hearing test booths and suites grason-stadler gsi 10 and gsi 16 microprocessor audiometer systems grason-stadler auto tympanometers gsi 27a and 28a grason-stadler middle ear analyzers model 33 versions 1 and 2 bio-logic evoked potential systems aep vep sep enn erg rep biocoustics simulators for audiometric and evoked potential training ics medical electronystagmographic systems industrial acoustics c o m p a n y (pty) limited p.o. box 14337 wadeville 1422 tel: (011) 902-4492 your contacts: hillary j reichenberg j a a p seymore we are only a phone call a w a y for further information on our products, maintenance and calibration service the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 5 3 single case experimental designs: an essential service in communicatively disabled care isabel c uys d phil (pretoria) department of speech pathology and audiology university of pretoria abstract a situation analysis of communication disabilities ofand services to this population in the rsa reveals a lack ofknowledge about the field and a paucity of research, probably due to therapists' extreme involvement in clinical practice. in this article the advantages of single case experimentation are putforward and specific designs are discussed in an attempt to motivate and enable clinicians to be producers of research. it is pointed out that this type of research will not only add valuable scientific information to the field of speech pathology and audiology, but it will also increase accountability in clinical practice. opsomming 'n situasie-ontleding van kommunikasie-afivykings van en dienste aan hierdie populasie in die rsa dui op 'ngebrek aan kennis op diegebied, sowel as 'n beperkte navorsingsuitset. lht kan waarskynlik toegeskryf word aan die terapeute se ekstreme betrokkenheid by die kliniese praktyk. in hierdie artikel word die voordele van enkelgeval eksperimentering uiteengesit. spesifieke ontwerpe word voorgehou in 'n poging om terapeute te inspireer om navorsingsbeoefenaars te word. hierdie tipe navorsing lewer nie slegs waardevolle inligting vir spraakheelkunde en oudiologie nie, maar help ook om die toerekenbaarheid van die kliniese praktyk te verhoog. introduction the report of the inquiry into the circumstances of disabled people in the republic of south africa reveals inter alia that about 12,5% of the total population is disabled and it includes the recommendations that the quality of services should be improved and that research with a view to the improvement of services should be conducted (department of national health and population development, 1987). unfortunately, due to the paucity of services, trained personnel are swamped with medical and rehabilitative services mostly diagnosing and treating patients on an individual basis. in a situation such as this, research is limited to a minimum, and research involving large groups of subjects, a near impossibility. a solution to this problem can, however, be found in the utilisation of the potential of clinicians, not only as consumers of research, but as producers of single case research. during their daily routine these professionals come into contact with potential subjects for research, but as has been pointed out, on an individual basis. these circumstances should be exploited for single case experimentation. single case experimental designs: advantages and limitations the results of single case study approaches have been used and quoted for many years usually with questionable credibility due to subjectivity and lack of experimental control. mcreynolds and thompson (1986:195) suggest that "a better role for case studies is description and identification of potential variables to be evaluated in experimental studies". this is especially applicable to the rsa context. because of the diversity of handicapped people, particularly from different language and cultural backgrounds, many unusual problems are encountered. the study of unique cases,will under these circumstances be of particular benefit to the science of communication pathology. single cases can be used in a number of different research designs. in the field of speech pathology and audiology single cases are often studied implementing a descriptive or even analytic survey design. these are, however, not experimental designs. a single case design can also be an experimental design, the socalled cause and effect method, the pretest-posttest control group design, or the laboratory method. "in its simplest form, the experimental method attempts to control the entire research situation, except for certain input variables which then become suspect as the cause of whatever change has taken place within the investigative design" (leedy, 1980:211). such data obtained from these studies are functual, objective and reliable. the question at this stage is: how can single case experimentation be utilised as a research input and as a service to the disabled? theoretically these studies are valuable in measuring the effectiveness of treatment variables, of describing disorders, or identifiable components of disorders over long periods of time, of evaluating the effects of disorders on the resulting disability, and of ultimately describing subgroups of disorders. single case experimentation can also be implemented in the identification of all the different variables related to or responsible for human development be it normal or pathological. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 isabel c uys one of the greatest practical advantages of single subject experimentation is the fact that the clinician can do research without sacrificing clinical intervention. in diagnosis the clinician identifies the behaviours to be modified (the dependent variables) and then selects specific procedures for treatment (the independent variables). this automatically forms the basis of a single case experimental design. the experiment can also be conducted during ordinary therapy sessions without the expense of sophisticated apparatus. this is often impossible when large groups of subjects are involved. "individuals differ in every physical and psychological attribute ever studied" (lindgren, byrne & petrinovich, 1961:219). it is recognised that patients with a specific disorder are not necessarily homogeneous. they differ qualitatively, in degree of impairment, and with regard to responsiveness to a particular treatment variable. in group experimental research this variability is managed through statistical analysis, usually referring to averages which in the end conceals rather than clarifies individual variability. single subject experimentation must never be confused with the unscientific, unplanned observation of behaviour. the single subject experimental approach is a scientific method which requires meticulous planning in advance and strict control during the execution of the experimental phases. selection of a single case experimental design "research is not aimless, undirected activity... (it) demands a definite aggressive plan" (leedy, 1980:5). planning is the most important prerequisite for successful research. the planning and execution of the research is done within the framework of the scientific method, which is a means whereby insight into undiscovered truth is sought (leedy, 1980:82); a set of rules that can be used for describing events, explaining events, and predicting events (silverman, 1977:29); and which involves certain decision taking steps (mouton & marais, 1985:16, 22). planning a single case study involves the following decisiontaking steps: the selection of a research theme, or problem or question. the topic or theme should always be rephrased in terms of an answerable question. not all questions are equally answerable, and "one cannot get a clear answer to a vague question" (johnson, 1946:52). silverman (1977:62) quotes the following example: "is hypnosis effective in treating stuttering? (versus) is the post-hypnotic suggestion, 'you will not stutter anymore', effective in reducing stuttering frequency?" ferent kinds of single case experimental designs and the researcher must be sure that the appropriate design is selected for a specific study. this depends first of all on the research question and secondly on the nature of the data required. this will be dealt with in detail later on. the collection, organization and classification of the data. data is collected within a closed system of controlled conditions "an area sealed off by given parametric limitations" (leedy, 1980:85). factors which are critical to the research can be isolated and the nature of the variables can actually be determined by control. without control the data will be worthless. the analysis and interpretation of the data. accumulated data are only potentially meaningful. "the significance of the data depends upon the way in which the facts are regarded" (leedy, 1980:6). in analyzing and interpreting the data, n e w insights are discovered and new meanings are revealed. this would then give rise to further unexplored questions for future research. only after this planning has been done systematically, can the researcher select the design. in disabled care experimental designs are frequently used to explore the full range of intervention questions including the acquisition, generalization, and maintenance of behaviours for impaired individuals. a heuristic method of selecting an appropriate design to answer a given question, is, however, necessary. kevin kearns (1986:205) devised such a method,, providing a variety of design options which depend on factors such as the nature of the target behaviours, the setting in which a study is conducted, the availability of additional subjects, and other practical exigencies. the sequential arrangement of steps in this design selection process is given in table i and this taxonomy could be viewed as a general, organizational tool that is intended to facilitate an understanding of factors to consider in the selection and use of single case designs. although the columns are presented in the order of evaluation strategy, clinical research questions, selected design options; and basic considerations, it is suggested that in using this method, the researcher deals with them in the order of clinical research question, evaluation strategy, basic considerations,, and lastly selected design options. j the research question refers to the outcome of intervention | and the way in which intervention strategies influence behai viour modification, e.g.: is there a difference in behaviour with ι or without treatment? to what degree do separate com-' ponents of the treatment contribute to behaviour modification? the drafting of a research programme, including the conceptualization of theoretical issues and the operationalization of these concepts i n terms of measurable parameters. in the humanities, measurement poses a bigger problem than in the physical sciences. the above example does, however, illustrate how behaviour can be measured in this case by counting the frequency of the occurrence of stuttering. without a clear operationalization of the central concepts included in the research question, it is impossible to select a design, or collect and interpret data. the selection of an appropriate design. there are many dif\ after the research question has been asked, the researcher can state the nature of the strategy to be followed, e.g.: can the behaviour during treatment be compared with the same type of behaviour without treatment? ^ ^ at this stage the researcher should pay attention to some basic considerations, or factors which would influence the selection of the appropriate design, e.g.: if there is uncertainty about the reversibility of behaviour, an abab design should rather be replaced by a multiple baseline design. only after all these factors have been dealt with, can the researcher select the design most appropriate for a particular study. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 55 single case experimental designs: an essential service in communicatively disabled care t h e following figures i l l u s t r a t e t h e u s e of different d e s i g n o p t i o n s : 1. treatment no treatment strategies. t h e i n i t i a l p h a s e is a p e r i o d d u r i n g w h i c h b a s e l i n e m e a s u r e m e n t s a r e t a k e n . it is i m p o r t a n t t h a t b a s e l i n e m e a s u r e m e n t s c o n t i n u e u n t i l a c e r t a i n a m o u n t of s t a b i l i t y i n b e h a v i o u r is r e a c h e d . d u r i n g t h e s e c o n d p h a s e t h e t r e a t m e n t is i n s t a t e d a n d table 1. evaluation strategies, research questions, design options, and considerations for single-subject experimental designs. evaluation strategy treatment no treatment comparison clinical research question does treatment, with all of its components, result in improved performance relative to no treatment? selected design options withdrawal and reversal designs abab bab aba multiple baseline designs (m,b) across behaviours across settings across subjects basic considerations is the therapeutic effect likely to reverse following the withdrawal of treatment? are functional independent behaviours or settings available? component assessment treatment — pari son treatment comsuccessive level analysis relative to a treatment package, to what degree do separate components of treatment contribute to improvement? does the addition of a component to a treatment package facilitate treatment effectiveness? what is the relative effectivenss of two or more treatments? multiple probe technique (variation of mb) (horner & baer, 1978) interaction (reduction) bc-b-bc-b bc-b-bc-a-bc-b-bc does treatment result in acquisition of successive steps in a chaining sequence? does treatment effectively modify a single, gradually acquired behaviour? interaction (additive) b-bc-b-bc b-bc-b-a-b-bc-b alternating treatments design replicated crossover design (barlow, hayes, & nelson, 1984) multiple probe technique changing criterion design are functionally independent behaviours available? are long or continuous baselines impractical? can the components be examined alone and in combination with the treatment package? can replication be obtained across subjects? can the components be examined alone and in combination with the treatment package? can replication be obtained across subjects? can treatments be rapidly alternated for each subject? are multiple subjects or target behaviours available? can treatment be "crossed over"? are nearly equal phase lengths possible? are steps in the treatment sequence independent? are earlier steps prerequisite to acquiring later steps? will changes in the dependent variable correspond to changes in the criterion level? will the dependent variable stabilize at successively more stringent criterion levels? φ ca oc a ( b a s e l i n e ) β ( t r e a t m e n t ) / a (withdrawal) β (treatment) a t i m e fkearns, 1986 a series of m e a s u r e m e n t s a r e t a k e n i n o r d e r t o i n d i c a t e t h e m o d i f i c a t i o n of t h e b e h a v i o u r . t h i s is followed b y a p e r i o d of w i t h d r a w a l of t r e a t m e n t , w i t h t h e p r e s u m p t i o n t h a t a r e l a p s e i n b e h a v i o u r w i l l b e d e m o n s t r a t e d . m u l t i p l e m e a s u r e m e n t s w i l l i n d i c a t e t h i s c h a n g e . t h e n t r e a t m e n t is a g a i n i n s t a t e d , etc. i n t h i s d e s i g n m u l t i p l e m e a s u r e m e n t s a r e m a d e o n m o r e t h a n o n e s i m i l a r , b u t i n d e p e n d e n t b e h a v i o u r , d u r i n g t h e a p h a s e . t r e a t m e n t is t h e n a p p l i e d for o n l y o n e b e h a v i o u r , w h i l e t h e a p h a s e c o n t i n u e s for t h e s e c o n d b e h a v i o u r . o n c e a s t a b l e c h a n g e is o b t a i n e d i n t h e first b e h a v i o u r , t h e β p h a s e is i n s t a ted for t h e s e c o n d b e h a v i o u r . f i g u r e 1 : a b a s i c a b a b w i t h d r a w a l d e s i g n w i t h t h e a l t e r n a t i o n o f n o t r e a t m e n t a n d t r e a t m e n t p h a s e s . (mcrleynolds & t h o m p s o n , 1 9 8 6 : 1 9 9 ) t h i s ab a r r a n g e m e n t r e m o v e s t h e n e c e s s i t y of w i t h d r a w a l of t r e a t m e n t a n d c a n b e u s e d w h e n w i t h d r a w a l o r r e v e r s i n g is i m p o s s i b l e o r e v e n u n e t h i c a l . die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 isabel c uys 2. component assessment strategies behavior 1 ! i a | β / * * » a. . . behavior 2 a β j^r v ^ s m m h j time figure 2: a multiple baseline across behaviours design with an extended baseline phase for behaviour 2. (mcreynolds & thompson, 1986:199) bc bc follow-up 1 0 0 80 ο α h— 5 60 υ <υ σι c 4 0 « u <ΰ c. a r r' 1 1 , . 1 . , — 1 ... .1..... j._ p / u i x l._l_ i _1 1 1 \ i 1 . 1 1 _ 1 l_ v 1 1 1 1 1—1 • f » 1 1 2 4 6 8 10 12 14 16 18 20 22 24 26 28 30 32 s e s s i o n s / figure 3: an interaction (additive) design including the phases a-b-bc-b-bc and a follow-up period. (kearns, 1986:210) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) single case experimental designs: an essential service i n communicatively disabled care 57 this design is selected when the researcher wants to establish whether the addition of another component to a specific treatment package would facilitate progress or enhance the effectiveness of the treatment. once again baseline measurements are taken. during the first phase only one type of treatment is given, followed by a combination of that treatment and the additional treatment. the additional treatment is withdrawn, and again added. during these consecutive phases multiple measurements of the behaviour change should indicate when optimal change takes place. s u h i e c ! 2 baseline treatment these are only a few examples of how single case experimental designs can be employed in disabled care research. unfortunately, especially when dealing with people, murphy's law will come into operation. to counteract this contingency, the researcher can employ certain alternatives, which is a product of the flexibility inherent in the application of single subject experimental designs. building in insurance through flexibility every researcher should be creative in designing experiments. while it is true that specific designs are appropriate for ans» p i c t u r e s qo b i e c t s figure 4: an alternating treatments design i (kearns, 1986:211) 3. treatment treatment comparison strategies as the effectiveness of one type of treatment is compared to that of another, an initial baseline phase is not required. the incorporation of a baseline phase does, however, give additio; nal information on a no-treatment-treatment comparison. in this design the treatments to be compared are administered and alternated rapidly, e.g.', half of each session. unfortunately this design does not permit unequivocal conclusions about the effectiveness of each method of treatment, but it does provide a means of evaluating the relative effectiveness of different treatment methods for specific patients. 4. successive level analysis this is a most successful method to employ in developmental studies. it answers the question about whether treatment would effectively modify a single, gradually acquired behaviour. after the baseline phase, a specific criterion is set according to which the success of the behaviour is measured. once the patient succeeds, another criterion is again set. it is, however, necessary that the researcher is familiar with the successive steps in the acquisition of this specific behaviour pattern as each criterion would serve as a goal in the developmental pattern. zoo 2 1 6 0 so 8.0 • β.ο ε ± 4 . 0 s u b l e c t s.c. b a t a t a ! · i t r e a t m e n t | e x t i n c t i o n i 2 . 0 t — -ι —1 16 —r20 f i v e m i n u t e i n t e r v a l · —ι— 2 5 figure 5: the changing criterion design (kearns, 1986:212) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 isabel c uy wering specific questions and that research must be planned in such a way that control can be exercised throughout the project, there still is a certain amount of flexibility that can be utilized to provide for the unexpected to provide additional sources of control. compared to research involving large groups of subjects (where similar control strategies must be maintained across the group), single subject designs lend themselves to flexible application (connell & thompson, 1986:214-215). flexibility should never weaken the scientific nature of single subject research; it should be utilized as an insurance against lack of control while examining the relationship between independent and dependent variables. "controls for an independent variable (treatment) applied in single-subject designs can be created to fit the particular characteristics of the treatment. the controls, if properly arranged, can be changed in response to changes in treatment once an experiment is underway." (connell & thompson, 1986:215.) t w o general forms of flexibility can be utilized in single subject research, viz. a priori and ad hoc flexibility. a priori flexibility comes into operation during the selection-ofdesign stage, i.e., before the experiment is carried out. the maintenance of experimental control can be assured when more than one design is incorporated in a single study. "even if all planned sources of control are preserved throughout the study, the extra control is not wasted because it is a form of replication that would increase confidence in the results." (connell & thompson, 1986:220.) as aba and multiple baseline designs are particularly compatible, the researcher can for instance decide on this combination when there is uncertae reversibility of behaviours. this combination will also reveal a possible dependency of behaviours which were previously regarded as independent of one another, as baselines might change even before treatment is introduced for a specific behaviour. connell & thompson (1986:221) suggest the general principle that additional controls be built into all studies except those that examine highly predictable behaviours. ad hoc flexibility comes into operation while the experiment is being conducted. a study should be planned in such a way that the design can, be modified during one of the experimental phases. these modifications are made in response to developments that arise during the course of the experiment in an attempt to maintain control. "the within-subject nature of experimental control allows for a certain amount of freedom in the selection of control strategies ... the controls, if properly arranged, can be changed in response to changes in treatment once an experiment is underway ... it is this freedom in how independent variables and control can be implemented and changed ... that distinguishes single-subject from group designs." (connell & thompson, 1986:215.) this "response-guided" experimentation (edgington, 1983: 64-65) comes into operation when unexpected patterns occur in the data. without an extension of the original design, treatments cannot be studied in greater depth. when investigating the effectiveness of a treatment package, the researcher might decide to select a multiple baseline across subjects design implementing a reversal design as control. during the course of treatment it becomes clear that the independent variable proves to be effective for two of the three subjects only. the experimenter can then systematically change the treatment implementing an interaction design while maintaining the reversal design control that was originally implemented. this modification will allow the experimenter to determine which modifications of procedures are effective for an individual subject. connell & thompson (1986:223) summarize the benefits of flexibility in single case research as follows: "by using flexible designs, it would be possible to obtain detailed information about factors in existing treatments that have a variable effect, a weak effect, or even no effect on learning. by contrasting variable and ineffective factors with consistent, effective factors across treatments, individuals and behaviours, it may be possible to identify common attributes of factors that are related to effectiveness within a particular disorder area and possibly across areas." revealing new meaning as accumulated data are only potentially meaningful, the researcher needs to process the data in order to obtain greater insight into the nature and meaning of the data. one of the available tools for processing and interpretation is statistics. in disabled care research wants to evaluate and draw conclusions about behaviour change, and experimental, as well as therapeutic criteria, and invoked to evaluate data (risley, 1970:103-127). the experimental criterion refers to reliability. this criterion is met when the subject's behaviour changes reliably under specific experimental conditions. on the other hand, the therapeutic criterion is met when the level of behaviour change is such that the subject presents with adequate functioning in society, complying with the norm. evaluation and interpretation of data in terms of the experimental criterion include visual inspection and statistical analysis. visual inspection may seem completely subjective, but special data requirements, in terms of specific criteria (e.gj, trends in change during certain experimental phases) need to be met. statistical methods again present the researcher with replicable computational methods and rules for making deci] sions about the reliability of a particular experimental crite-j rion. 1 ι ι the applied or clinical significance, i.e., the therapeutic criterion, can be addressed by comparing the subject's post-experimental behaviour with the norm, o r by having various raters evaluate the magnitude of the behavioural change. interrater reliability then becomes an issue. although there are still sources of controversy, "statistical analyses in single case research may provide a valuable sup-. plement rather than an alternative to visual inspection" (kazdin, 1984:291) and a number of statistical tests can be applied to data obtained from a single case. / ' in a a-b-a-b design comparisons can be made of behaviour during the baseline and intervention phases (t-test), or analysis of variance (f test) can be made to compare the four phases. / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) single case experimental designs: an essential service in communicatively disabled care 59 if, however, the data are serially dependent, these tests may not be appropriate w h i c h w o u l d necessitate variations of these tests in order to reduce the effect of serial dependency. time series analyses can be used to compare behaviour change over time for a small group of subjects or e v e n a single subject. in single case research the time series analysis is advantageous in that a t-test is provided and important information o n the change i n level and slope is given during the different phases. randomization tests are used when treatments are assigned randomly to different occasions and as such are useful for evaluating data obtained from alternative treatment designs. if the design complies with the criterion of randomization and rapid alteration of experimental conditions, these tests provide a useful set of statistical techniques for single case research. the test of ranks (r) is used for evaluating data obtained in multiple baseline designs and requires that data be collected across several baselines, e.g., different behaviours, subjects, or settings the minimum requirement for detecting a statistically significant effect at the .05 level of confidence being four baselines. in the case of slow and gradual, or even fluctuating performance, the intervention can still be evaluated on the basis of mean performance, which is an advantage of this statistical procedure. a description of the rate of behaviour change over time is supplied by the split-middle technique. it reveals a linear trend in the data, characterizes present performance, and predicts future performance. "rate of behaviour" (frequency/time) has been advocated as the most useful measure for this method (kazdin, 1984:313). problems do exist in drawing inferences when using this method, but as a descriptive tool the splitmiddle technique provides valuable information about level and slope changes, that is otherwise seldom reported. on the issue of whether statistical tests should be used to draw inferences from single case research, kazdin (1984:321) concludes that statistical analyses does not necessarily conflict with single case designs or their purposes; that when applied research attempts to developja technology of behaviour change and to achieve clinically impjortant effects, statistical analyses will have limited value, but that there are several uses of statistics that may contribute to the goals of applied research. i conclusion i / the world health organization and the report of the inquiry into disability in south africa define rehabilitation as an effective, goal-oriented and time-limited process (department of national health and population development, 1987). with this implied emphasis on accountability in disabled care, personnel will find it increasingly advantageous to demonstrate scientifically the impacts of their clinical programmes on their patients/clients (silverman, 1977:xiii). the subcommittee on speech impairment stresses the importance of a research methodology applicable to the wide variety of multilingual, multicultural disabilities in the republic of south africa. a situation analysis of the state of disability care reveals inter alia that there are insufficient services and insufficient knowledge about the communicatively disabled in this complex society. while these facts stress the need for basic and applied research, the employment of typical subject designs (group designs) for the evaluation of applied behavioural interventions is rendered impossible the primary task of speechlanguage-hearing therapists being the assessment and rehabilitation of the communicatively disordered, usually on an individual basis. the value of research with large populations should not be underestimated, especially because of the advantage of generalization. but, as a solution to a variety of practical problems, viz. the one-to-one relationship in therapy, and individual differences, single case experimentation should be utilized more extensively. single case experimental designs are presented as a methodology particularly applicable to the needs of disabled care. it provides data concerning the "typical" behaviour of an individual subject under experimental conditions, allows for the evaluation of intervention strategies applied to a specific individual, and creates the possibility to generalize to the population from which the subject was selected, while not on a statistical, then on a logical basis (silverman, 1977:67). the research potential of health care personnel should be tapped by bringing the advantages of single case experimental research to their attention. ultimately this will be to the benefit of the disabled. bibliography connell, p.j. & thompson, c.k. flexibility of single-subject experimental designs. part iii: using flexibility to design or modify experiments, journal of speech and hearing disorders, 51, 214-225, 1986. department of national health and population development. disability in the republic of south africa, vol 1 & 10, 1987. edgington, e.s. response-guided exx>eximentati(m.contemporary psychology, 28, 64-65, 1983. johnson, w. people in quandaries, ny: harper & row, p. 52-53 1946. kazdin, a.e. statistical analyses for single-case experimental designs. in: barlow, d.h. & hersen, m. single case experimental designs. new york: pergamon press, 1984. kearns, k.p. flexibility of single-subject experimental designs. part ii: design selection and arrangement of experimental phases, journal of speech and hearing^disorders, 51, 204-213, 1986. leedy, p.d. practical research, 3rd ed., new york: macmillan publishing company, 1980. lindgren, h.c., byrne, d., petrinovich, l. psychology: an introduction to a behavioural science, 2nd ed., new york, john wiley & sons, inc., 1961. mcreynolds, l.v. & thompson, c.k. flexibility of single-subject experimental designs. part i: review of the basics of singlesubject designs, journal of speech and hearing disorders, 51, _ 194-203, 1986. mouton, j. & marais, h.c. metodologie van die geesteswetenskappe. pretoria: raad vir geesteswetenskaplike navorsing, 1985. risley, t.r. behaviour modification: an experimental-thera-peutic endeavour. in hamerlynck, l.a., davidson, p.o., & acker, l.e. (eds.), behaviour modification and ideal health services, canada, university of calgary press, 1970. silverman, f.h. research design in speech pathology and audiology. new jersey: prentice-hall, inc., 1977. diesuid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 0 information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, or critically evaluative theoretical, or therapeutic issues dealing with disorders of speech, voice, hearing or language, or on aspects of the processes underlying these. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material, which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. all contributions are reviewed by at least two consultants who are not provided with author identification. form of manuscript. authors should submit four neatly typewritten manuscripts in triple spacing with wide margins which should not exceed much more than 25 pages. each page should be numbered. the first page of two copies should contain the title of the article, name of author/s, highest degree and address or institutional affiliation. the first page of the remaining two copies should contain only the title of the article. the second page of all copies should contain only an abstract'^ 100 words) which should be provided in both english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided —for overseas contributors. all paragraphs should start at the left margin and not be indented. -major headings, where applicable, should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements and references. tables and figures should be prepared on separate sheets (one per table/figure). figures, graphs and line drawings must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper. lettering appearing on tl êe should be uniform and professionally done, bearing in mind » that such lettering should be legible after a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should not be included in the illustration but should appear below it. the titles of tables and figures should be concise but explanatory. the title of tables appears above, and of figures below. tables and figures should be numbered in order of appearance (with arabic numerals). the amount of tabular and illustrative material allowed will be at the discretion of the editor (usually not more than 6). references. references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and date, e.g. van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, et al. after the first author will suffice. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list. references should be listed alphabetically in triple-spacing at the end of the article. for acceptable abbreviations of names of journals, consult the fourth issue (october) of dsh abstracts or the world list of scientific periodicals. the number of references used should not exceed much more than 20. note the following examples: locke, j.l. clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord, 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology, 3rd ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. proofs. galley proofs will be sent to the author wherever possible. corrections other than typographical errors will be charged to'the author. reprints. 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge.' all manuscripts and correspondence should be addressed to: the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech and hearing association, p.o. box 31782, braamfontein 2017, south africa. inligting vir bydraers die siiid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer verslae en artikels oor navorsing, of krities evaluerende artikels oor die teoretiese of terapeutiese aspekte van spraak-, stem-, gehoorof taalafwykings, of oor aspekte van die prosesse onderliggend aan hierdie afwykings. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings sal nie materiaal aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. alle bydraes word deur minstens twee konsultante nagegaan wat nie ingelig is oor die identiteit van die skrywer nie. formaat van die manuskrip. skrywers moet vier netjies getikte manuskripte in 3-spasi'ering en met bree kantlyn indien, en dit moet nie veel langer as 25 bladsye wees nie. elke bladsy moet genommer wees. op die eerste bladsy van 2 afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, die naam van die skrywer/s, die hoogste graad behaal en die adres of naam van hulle betrokke instansie verskyn. op die eerste bladsy van die oorblywende twee afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskyn. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. alle paragrawe moet teenaan die linkerkantlyn begin word en moet nie ingekeep word nie. hoofopskrifte moet, waar dit van toepassing is, in die volgende volgorde wees: metode, resultate, bespreking, gevolgtrekking, erkennings en verwysings. tabelle en figure moet op afsonderlike bladsye verskyn (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie). figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge moet oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte gedoen word. letterwerk wat hierop verskyn moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50%-verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by die illustrasie moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verklarings of omskrywings moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maardaaronder verskyn. die byskrifte van tabelle moet bo-aan verskyn en die van figure onderaan. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word (met arabiese syfers). die hoeveelheid materiaal in die vorm van tabelle en illustrasies wat toegelaat word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). verwysings. verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, bv. van riper (1971). waar daar meer as twee skrywers is, sal et al. na die eerste skrywer voldoende wees'. die name van alle skrywers moet in die verwysingslys verskyn. verwysings moet alfabeties in 3-spasi'ering aan die einde van die artikel gerangskik word. vir die aanvaarde afkortings van tydskrifte se. titels, raadpleeg die vierde uitgawe (oktober) van dsh abstracts of the world list of scientific periodicals. die getall verwysings wat gebruik is, moet nie veel meer as 20 wees nie. ' let op die volgende voorbeelde: ι locke, j.l. clinical phonology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord, 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology, 3de ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice hall, 1971. proewe. galeiproewe sal waar moontlik aan die skrywer gestuur word. die onkoste van veranderings, behalwe tipografiese foute, sal deur die skrywer self gedra moet word. herdrukke. 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis'verskaf word. alle manuskripte en korrespondensie moet gerig word aan: die redakteur, / die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings. die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraaken gehoorheelkunde, posbus 31782, braamfontein 2017, suid-afrika. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die keuring van eerstejaarstudente in die departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie (universiteit van pretoria) isabel c uys dphil (pret) erna alant ma(log) (pret) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming die artikel handel oor die keuring van logopedika-studente aan die universiteit van pretoria en beskryf die kompleksiteit van hierdieprosedure. die huidige benaderingpoog om studente te selekteer wat die kursus suksesvol kan voltooi, met die aanname dat 'n toegewyde student wel deur die regte supervisie gelei kan word tot 'n volwaardige terapeut. gerekenariseerde data illustreer sekere verbande tussen studenteprestasies in die finale jaar en inisiele keuringtellings. hierdie inligting word gei'nterpreteer in terme van die tekortkominge sowel as sterk punte van so η benadering. abstract this article deals with the selection of speech therapy students at the university of pretoria and describes the complexity of this process. the present approach attempts to select students that have potential to successfully complete the course, with the assumption that with appropriate supervision, these students can be guided to become competent speech therapists. computerized data illustrates certain correlations between students' achievements in the final year and initial selection scores. this information is interpreted in terms of the short-comings as well as strengths of this approach. meeste van die gevestigde professionele kursusse word op die een of die ander tyd gekonfronteer met 'n probleemtoestand wat keuring van voornemende studente noodsaak. by^die universiteit van pretoria, soos seker ook by ander universiteite die geval is, is die uitsoek van die beste studente vir een bepaalde kursus ten koste van ander kursusse 'n vreemde gedagte. in die geval van die β log-kursus het verskeie faktore daartoe aanleiding gegee dat keuring oorweeg moes word, nl. — studentetalle het vanaf 1971-1976 aansienlik toegeneem, te wete 'n toename van 109,1%! by eerstejaarstudente en 92,2 % by alle jaargroepe tesame in die departement. — die fisiese opleidingsfasiliteite kon nie in der mate uitgebrei en aangepas word om al die studente, veral met betrekking tot praktiese opleidingsgeriewe, sinvol te akkommodeer nie. — 'n bykomende faktor waarvan kennis geneem moes word, is die wyse waarop natuurlike seleksie wel gemanifesteer het. in die periode vanaf 1971-1975 het gemiddeld slegs 50% van die studente gekwalifiseer. met die toename in studentetalle is daar ook 'n beduidende afname in die slaagsyfer bemerk. in 'n poging om die vereiste akademiese en praktiese standaarde te handhaaf, is dus besluit om 'n ondersoek te loods na die wenslikheid van keuring. 'n vasgestelde inname sou kwaliteitskontrole verseker en die natuurlike seleksie (staking, druipdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 syfer, ens.) laat afneem. kandidate met die grootste potensiele kans op akademiese sukses sou toegelaat word tot die kursus. hierdie oorwegings geld dan ook by verskeie kursusse ter plaatse en oorsee. (hilson en mckendrick 1978; furler-kools 1981) beskrywing van die keuringsprosedures die afdeling universiteitsonderwysbeplanning van die studentediensburo in samewerking met die departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie het 'n deeglike veldverkenning gedoen met die oog op die samestelling van 'n bevredigende keuringsmeganisme. daar bestaan werklik min navorsingsgegewens oor keuring. uit alle beskikbare studies blyk dit egter dat keurders met een grondliggende probleem te doen kry, nl. word studente gekeur met die oog op die suksesvolle voltooiing van hulle opleiding, of met die oog op sukses in die toekomstige praktyk? (furlerkools 1981; hilson en mckendrich 1978). anders gestel: moet goeie studente of goeie terapeute geselekteer word? hierdie dilemma word gekompliseer deur die gebrek aan wetenskaplike bewyse oor die verband tussen opleidingsondervinding en praktykvereistes (malan 1979). dit lyk asof opvoedkundige en praktykdoelwitte selfs in sekere opsigsasha 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 isabel c uys and erna alant te van mekaar kan verskil. hilson en mckendrick (1978) brei op hierdie verskille uit waar dit betrekking het op die maatskaplike werk. indien die potensieel-suksesvolle student gesoek word sal daar bewyse van akademiese vermoens en intellektuele kapasiteit moet wees. daarteenoor, sal die potensieelsuksesvolle maatskaplike werker sekere persoonlikheidseienskappe moet openbaar, bv. volwassenheid en emosionele stabiliteit. hierdie bevindings is eweneens van toepassing op die spraakterapeut-oudioloog. matriekresultate (akademiese en intellektuele vermoens) blyk nog steeds die beste indikator van sukses op die tersiere onderwysvlak te wees. dit is 'n sterk motivering vir keuring op grand van akademiese prestasie. ongelukkig is dit so dat die mens nie voorspelbaar is nie. geen keuringstelsel kan ooit waterdig wees nie. soos in die geval van maatskaplike werk (hilson en mckendrick 1978) kan mens alleenlik hoop dat die student na 'n versigtig-beplande opleiding, ook in die praktyk sukses sal bereik. dit is juis hierdie aanname wat die keuring van verskeie kursusse (malan 1979; hilson en mckendrick 1978 en staf van die opleiding logopedie, hoensbroek 1981) rig. uit die voorafgaande blyk dit dat verskeie vrae moes beantwoord word voordat 'n sinvolle keuringsprosedure opgestel kon word: — watter eienskappe word gesoek in die voornemende β logstudent? — watter prosedure sal hierdie eienskappe uitlig? — wanneer moet keuring plaasvind? — deur wie moet die keuring hanteer word? aan die hand van beginselbesluite in die beantwoording van die bogenoemde vrae, het die belang van die volgende na vore gekom, nl. akademiese en intellektuele vermoens, spesifieke persoonlikheidseienskappe (aanleg, menslike dienslewering, motivering, ens.) asook normale spraak, stem en gehoor. met die oog hierop is op die volgende keuringsprosedures besluit: aantal studente met inagneming van beskikbare opleidingsfasiliteite is besluit op 'n maksimum van 25 studente per jaar. eerste keuring die eerste keuring vind teen die einde van die jaar plaas en word gegrond op die kandidaat se prestasie in die rekordeksamen, omdat die eindeksamenpunt nog nie beskikbaar is nie. hiervolgens word 50 kandidate tot die finale keuring toegelaat. finale keuring die finale keuring vind gedurende januarie plaas en in hierdie geval word gebruik gemaak van akademiese prestasie, groeptoetse, 'n persoonlike onderhoud, sowel as 'n evaluasie van die kandidaat se spraaken gehoorvermoens. akademiese prestasie daar is in beginsel besluit om die studente wat die grootste kans op sukses in die kursus het, te selekteer. die finale keursyfer vir akademiese prestasie (met 'n gewig van 50%) word bereken van die gemiddelde matrikulasie-eindeksamenpunt plus kredietpunte (vir vakke op die hoer graad geslaag, asook enige toepaslike universiteitsen ander kursusse), minus strafpunte (vir universiteitsen ander kursusse wat gedruip is). aanleg en ander toetse omdat daar besluit is dat sekere eienskappe 'n voorvereiste is van suksesvolle voltooiing van die β log-kursus, is besluit om die volgende groeptoetse af te neem. al die toetse saam dra 'n gewig van 30% by tot die totale keursyfer. nuwe suid-afrikaanse groeptoets (rgn 1965) die voile skaal van 6 subtoetse (3 verbaal en 3 nie-verbaal) word toegepas om 'n beeld van die kandidaat se algemene intellektuele vermoens te verkry. die totale roupunt word met die oog op 'n keurpunt uit 10 verwerk volgens riglyne soos neergele deur die studentediensburo (u p). senior aanlegtoets (rgn 1975) die noodsaaklikheid om sekere eienskappe by voornemende studente te identifiseer word deur verskillende opleidingsinstansies beklemtoon. (furler-kools 1981; staf van die opleiding logopedie, hoensbroek 1981). aanleg verwys in hierdie toets na spesifieke verstandelike vermoens wat die persoon in staat stel om sekere vaardighede en bekwaamhede te ontwikkel. hierdie toets dek 'n baie wye veld en alleenlik drie items is toepaslik gereken by die keuring van β log-studente, nl. verbale begrip, woordbou en geheue (paragraaf). hierdie items is aanduidend van die kandidaat se algemene peil van kognitiewe funksionering, redeneringsvermoe, assosiasievlotheid en geheue. ook hierdie roupunte word volgens 'n tabel tot 'n keurpunt verwerk. sestienfaktor-persoonlikheidsvraelys (cattel, eber en tatsoeka 1970) hierdie baie omvattende toets meet sestien afsonderlike persoonlikheidsdimensies waarvolgens uitsprake in verband met verwagte prestasie, beroepsgerigtheid, leierskapeienskappe en selfs moontlike neurotiese neigings gemaak kan word. vir die doel van die keuring is slegs vyf van die faktore as toepaslik beskou, nl. intelligensie, emosionele rypheid, pligsgetrouheid, skuldgeneigdheid en gespannendheid (d.i. faktore b, d, g,jo en q4). ook hierdie roupunte word tot 'n keurpunt verwerk. die resultate van hierdie drie toetse gesamentlik stel die keurkomitee in staat om te verseker dat die kandidaat die regte kursus kies, omdat spesifieke vermoens, algemene kognitiewe vermoens en persoonlikheidseienskappe ondersoek word. hierdie gesamentlike punt dra dan die gewig van 30% by tot die finale keursyfer. persoonlike onderhoud / die persoonlike onderhoud dra alleenlik 'n gewig van 20% by tot die finale keursyfer. die feit dat die onderhoudsvoerthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die keuring van eerstejaarstudente in die departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie (universiteit van pretoria) 31 ders opgeleide personeel met ervaring is, lei nie noodwendig tot intraen interbeoordelaar betroubaarheid nie. daarby duur elke onderhoud ongeveer 10-15 minute, wat 'n baie kort tyd is om aspekte soos gemotiveerdheid en belangstelling, kommunikasieen taaluitdrukkingsvermoe, tweetaligheid en mensgerigte dienslewering te evalueer. 'n ander aspek wat egter in 'n persoonlike kontaksituasie hanteer word, is die ondersoek van spraaken gehoorvermoens. indien enige sodanige belemmerings die kandidaat se prestasie tydens die opleiding nadelig sal be'invloed, word die kandidaat afgekeur, afgesien van enige ander keursyfers. hierdie keuringsprosedure word vanaf 1977 gevolg. die eerste gekeurde studente het dus in 1980 gekwalifiseer en daar is besluit dat opvolgingsnavorsing 'n aanvang kan neem, hoewel die steekproef nog relatief klein is. die belang van langtermynnavorsing, asook korrelasies tussen prestasie op keuring en sukses in die praktyk word besef. huidiglik is dit egter alleenlik moontlik om navorsing en evaluasie van die keuringsprosedures te rig op finalejaarstudente se prestasie. tabel 1 steekproefbeskrywing jaar herhalers nil we gekwalifiseerde jaar herhalers kandidate terapeute 1977 25 18 1978 2 23 15 1979 2 23 18 totaal 4 71 51 'n totaal van 51 studente is dus by die studie betrek. dataverwerking die data is gerekenariseer waarna beskrywende statistiek sowel as verskeie korrelasies tussen veranderlikes bereken is. 'n inferensie insake die korrelasiekoeffisient het verskeie liniere verbande tussen sekere veranderlikes bevestig. volgens hierdie metode word 'n p-waarde kleiner as 0,05 gei'nterpreteer as betekenisvol (steyn, smit, du toit 1984). evaluasie metode van evaluasie die keuringsprosedure is geevalueer aan die hand van twee aspekte: i vergelyking van die slaagsyfer by kwalifikasie van spraakterapeute vyf jaar voor keuring ingestel is, en drie jaar nadat keuring ingestel is. hoewel keuring nie hoofsaaklik ingestel word om die slaagsyfer by kwalifikasie te verhoog nie, kan die gegewens moontlik ook 'n aanduiding gee van die algemene toewyding van die studente aan hul studies, ii assosiasies van keuringsyfers en prestasiesyfers (akademies sowel as prakties) van finalejaar-(kwalifiserende) spraakterapiestudente. ideaal gesproke sou die evaluasie van 'n keuringsprosedure slegs kon geskied deur middel van die vergelyking van gekeurde en ongekeurde studente in dieselfx d e beroep. aangesien laasgenoemde nie in die situasie prakties uitvoerbaar is nie, sou 'n mens die gekeurde studente se vordering in die spraakterapiekursus noukeurig kon dophou en telkens prestasie van die studente vergelyk met die aanvanklike keuringstellings. hilson et al (1978) het by voorbeeld keuringstellings vergelyk met die prestasietellings van maatskaplike werkstudente aan die einde van hul eerste jaar. aangesien die eerste jaar in die spraakterapiekursus egter baie inleidend van aard is (bv. baie beperkte prakties word gedoen), is gevoel dat die waarde van keuring by spraakterapie meer sigbaar kan word wanneer die keuringstellings vergelyk word met die kwalifiserende prestasies. dit is dan ook teen hierdie agtergrond dat daar besluit is om gebruik te maak van die finalejaar-prestasietellings en die te vergelyk met keuringstellings. 1 resultate vergelyking van slaagsyfer (aan die einde van die finale jaar) gedurende vyf jaar voor keuring (sien tabel 2) en die slaagsyfer gedurende drie jaar na keuring ingestel is (sien tabel 3) tabel 2 ongekeurde finalejaarstudentesyfers jaar aantal eerste jaars aantal gekwalifiseer fersentasie gekwalifiseer 1971 33 11 33 1972 31 27 87 1973 39 21 54 1974 55 22 40 1975 53 19 36 totaal 211 100 50 tabel 3 gekeurde finalejaarstudentesyfers jaar aantal eerste jaars aantal gekwalifiseer ptrsentasie gekwalifiseer 1977 25 18 72 1978 25 15 60 1979 25 18 72 totaal 75 51 68 steekproef alle gekeurde studente (tussen 1977 er. 1979) wat gekwalifiseer het (tussen 1981 en 1983) is by die studie ingesluit (sien tabel 1). ' uit bogenoemde tabelle blyk dit dat die gemiddelde slaagsyfer by kwalifikasie toegeneem het met 18% sedert keuring ingestel is. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 isabel c uys and erna alant assosiasies vir keuringsyfers en die prestasie by f1nalejaarstudente korrelasiematriks vir assosiasies van keuringsyfers en die prestasie by finalejaarstudente spraak heelkunde oudiologie kombineer spraakheelkunde en oudiologie aanlegtoetse onderhoud matriekprestasie finale keuringspunt praktiese jaarpunt -0.20881 0.25940 0.25077 0.09699 0.1414 0.0660 0.0759 0.4984 teoriejaarpunt 0.01871 0.15535 0.57103 0.42570 0.8963 0.2763 0.0001 0.0018 totale jaarpunt -0.11859 0.25191 0.48316 0.30761 0.4112 0.0745 0.0003 0.0281 eksamenpunt 0.02520 0.23497 0.34233 0.30932 0.8607 0.0970 0.0139 0.0272 finale punt -0.02649 0.25633 0.42208 0.32976 0.8536 0.0694 0.0020 0.0181 praktiese jaarpunt -0.35374 0.09118 0.19728 -0.07846 0.0109 0.5246 0.1652 0.5842 teoriejaarpunt -0.20149 0.06024 0.37900 0.15814 0.1562 0.6745 0.0061 0.2677 totale jaarpunt -0.31969 0.09862 0.34480 0.05938 0.0222 0.4911 0.0132 0.6790 eksamenpunt -0.04961 0.20213 0.48404 0.35022 0.7296 0.1549 0.0003 0.0118 finale punt -0.16401 0.16847 0.50310 0.28122 0.2501 0.2373 0.0002 0.0456 praktiese jaarpunt -0.31179 0.20067 0.25249 0.01471 0.0259 0.1580 0.0739 0.9184 teoriejaarpunt -0.10269 0.12145 0.53497 0.22890 0.4733 0.3959 0.0001 0.0184 totale jaarpunt -0.23606 0.18977 0.44786 0.19883 0.0954 0.1823 0.0010 0.1619 eksamenpunt -0.01191 0.24445 0.45753 0.36681 0.9339 0.0838 0.0007 0.0081 finale punt -0.09709 0.22793 0.48850 0.32556 0.4979 0.1077 0.0003 0.0197 die volgende gegewens (sien tabel 5 en 6) kan uit tabel 4 onttrek word ter verduideliking van betrokke assosiasies. tabel 5 betekenisvolle assosiasies tussen teoretiese en keuringsyfers tabel 6 betekenisvolle assosiasies tussen praktiese jaarpunt en keuringsyfers j keuringsprosedure spraakheelkunde oudiologie gekombineerd aanlegtoetse _ onderhoud matriekprestasie 0,0001* 0,0160* 0,0001* finale keuringspunt 0,0042* 0,0559* keuringsprosedure spraakheelkunde oudiologie i gekombineerd aanlegtoetse -0.0308* -0,0830* onderhoud matriekprestasie finale keuringspunt dit blyk duidelik uit voorafgaande dat matriekresultate die enigste veranderlike is wat 'n betekenisvolle verband vertoon met die teoretiese punte gedurende die jaar. dieselfde neiging is ook waarneembaar wanneer die eksamenpunte inaggeneem word in tabel 4. uit die bogenoemde blyk dit dat 'n negatiewe betekenisvolle verband bestaan tussen resultate van die aanlegtoetse en oudiologie prakties, asook die gekombineerde praktiese punt. geen verdere betekenisvolle assosiasies is duidelik nie. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die keuring van eerstejaarstudente in die departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie (universiteit van pretoria) 33 beskrywende stat1stiek met betrekking tot die onderskeie keur1ngsaspekte tabel 7 gemiddelde telling en standaard afwyking van keuringsaspekte gemiddeld standaard afwyking aanlegtoetse onderhoud matriekprestasie • finale keuringspunt 20.54901961 14.78235294 37.12941176 72.29019608 3.39596069 1.79105621 3.88197342 6.12929165 uit voorafgaande tabel blyk dit dat die 'onderhoud' in die prosedure die kleinste standaard afwyking toon, met ander woorde, die kleinste variasie random die gemiddelde. laasgenoemde is interessant aangesien dit impliseer dat hierdie groep meer homogeen was wat betref hulle tellings op hierdie keuringsaspek. bespreking matriekprestasie was die mees konstante indikator van teoretiese prestasie van die studente in oudiologie sowel as spraakheelkunde. hierdie bevinding is ook gestaaf in hilson et al. (1978) se studie op maatskaplike werkstudente. wat egter effe verontrustend is, is die neiging wat betref die korrelasie tussen die praktiese punte en keuringspunte. die enigste betekenisvolle verband gevind in hierdie korrelasie is tussen aanlegtoetse en oudiologie-prakties, en hierdie verband is 'n negatiewe verband. hoewel so 'n bevinding oenskynlik verbasend kon wees, kan dit moontlik verklaar word aan die hand van die betrokke toetse wat ingesluit is onder die kategorie 'aanlegtoetse'. die aanlegtoetspunt word verkry deur die som van gegewens op drie toetse wat elk op 'n tienpuntskaal beoordeel word. hierdiedrie toetse gekombineer dra by tot 30% van die keuringstelling. die toetse is die volgende: senior aanlegtoets (sat), nuwe suid-afrikaanse groepstoets (nsag), persoonlikheidsvraelys (16-pf slegs vyf van die sestien faktore word inaggeneem by keuring: intelligensie, pligsgetrouheid, skuldgeneigdheid, gespannendheid en emosionele rypheid). wanneer hierdie toetse van nader beskou word blyk dit dat daar 'n baie groot klem is op die meting van intellektuele vermoens van kandidate. die sat sowel as die nsagt meet kognitiewe funksionering, spesifiek en algemeen, met ander woorde, ten minste 20 % van die aanlegtoetspunt word verteenwoordig deur 'n kognitiewe metingspunt. in hierdie lig gesien, sou 'n mens nie noodwendig 'n positiewe korrelasie verwag tussen praktiese werk en aanlegtoetse nie aangesien daar nie noodwendig 'n noue verband bestaan tussen praktiese vaardigheid en intellektuele of akademiese vermoens nie (hilson et al 1978). terselfdertyd kan die voorafgaande argument nie gesien word as 'n verklaring van 'n betekenisvolle negatiewe verband tussen intellektuele vermoens (soos hier getoets) en praktiese punte nie. die grootte van die huidige steekproef kon inderdaad 'n faktor gewees het by hierdie bevinding. dit sou dus onvanpas wees om hierdie resultate as allesomvattend te beskryf. nog 'n interessante waarneming wat gemaak kan word, is die afwesigheid van enige verband tussen die 'onderhoud' tydens keuring en die teoretiese en praktiese punte. wanneer die standaard afwyking vir die groep se onderhoudtellings beskou word, word dit duidelik dat die gekeurde studente baie dieselfde punte behaal het in die onderhoud. hieruit kan moontlik die volgende afleidings gemaak word: dat alle kandidate dieselfde eienskappe gehad het met betrekking tot die aspekte gedek in die onderhoud (motivering en belangstelling kommunikasie en taaluitdrukking, mensgerigte dienslewering en algemene ingesteldheid en voorkoms); dat die vier aspekte gedek in die onderhoud te wyd gedefinieer is en derhalwe nie individuele verskille reflekteer nie. hilson et al (1978) het wel 'n betekenisvolle verband tussen die onderhoud in seleksie en finale eerstejaarspunte gevind. mens sou moontlik hierdie verband kon verklaar in terme van die relatiewe kort tyd wat verloop het tussen die keuringstyd (met ander woorde, die begin van die jaar) en die eksamen aan die einde van die jaar. blootstelling aan die kursus tydens die eerste jaar is inleidend van aard en kan dus nie in werklikheid gesien word as 'n betroubare maatstaf vir die evaluasie van die keuringsprosedure nie. dit is nie noodwendig te se dat alle studente wat die eerste jaar slaag die kursus gaan voltooi nie. die huidige studie het dus eerder die finalejaarprestasie geneem as maatstaf. dit impliseer egter dat daar 'n vier jaar verloop is tussen die 'onderhoud' (of enige ander keuringstelling) en die prestasiepunt. gedurende hierdie tyd word die studente nie alleenlik blootgestel aan die terapeutiese situasie nie, maar vereis die kursus dat studente emosionele groei toon om steeds meer veeleisende pasiente in die terapiesituasie te kan hanteer. dit is dus nie heeltemal verrassend wanneer die huidige bevindings toon dat daar geen betekenisvolle verband bestaan tussen die aanvanklike onderhoudspunte en finalejaarprestasie nie. laasgenoemde kan gemotiveer word deurdat die onderhoud soos gedefinieer in 'n vorige paragraaf nie potensiele emosionele groei uitlig nie, maar wel klem le op die student se belangstelling en ingesteldheid tydens die onderhoud. die fokus in keuring is dus op die seleksie van studente wat die opleiding suksesvol kan voltooi, eerder as die keuring van studente in terme van hul potensiaal vir toekomstige praktyk (hilson et al 1978). dit is veral duidelik wanneer gekyk word na die keuringsprosedure in terme van kognitiewe funksionering, matriekprestasie, ingesteldheid en belangstelling tydens aansoek. die aanname wat hier gemaak word, is dat die gekeurde student gelei kan word om 'n goeie terapeut te word onder bevoegde leiding. dit impliseer dat die proses van kliniese supervisie tydens opleiding baie sterk benadruk word (oratio 1977; lewis 1983). soos blyk uit die bespreking, is die konsep van 'keuring' voorwaar kompleks. dit spruit nie net uit die probleme by die seleksie van faktore wat ingesluit moet word in die keuring nie, maar ook uit die dilemma van wanneer die beste tyd sou wees om die effektiwiteit van die keuringsprosedure te toets, asook die keuse van maatstawwe waaraan keuringseffektiwiteit gemeet kan word bo en behalwe akademiese prestasie. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 isabel c uys and erna alant hoewel daar baie praktiese probleme met die keuring van studente bestaan, is dit egter belangrik dat hierdie aspekte gesien moet word binne die raamwerk van die persentasie van studente wat uiteindelik kwalifiseer. soos voorheen genoem is daar 'n-duidelike styging in die persentasie van spraakterapeute wat kwalifiseer sedert keuring ingestel is. hoewel keuring nie gerig is op die verhoging van die slaagsyfer in 'n kursus nie, maar eerder die selektering van studente wat die beste bydrae tot die beroep sou kon maak, kan 'n verhoging in slaagsyfer wel 'n maatstaf wees in die meting van betrokkenheid van die student by die kursus. binne hierdie bree raamwerk sou die proses van keuring wel baie nuttig en relevant wees, hoewel daar noodwendig altyd krities gekyk sal word na die inhoud en wyse van die keuringsproses. dit is teen hierdie agtergrond dat sekere veranderinge in die bestaande keurings-prosedure gemaak kan word. hoewel keuring gerig is op die seleksie van studente wat die spraakterapiekursus suksesvol sal voltooi, is dit nodig vir die keurders om sensitief te wees vir indikatore wat groeipotensiaal by studente kan aandui. voorbeelde van sulke indikatore is die volgende: — aanlegtoetse soos hier gedefinieer, sou moontlik aangepas kan word om minder klem te plaas op die kognitiewe vermoens van die individue, ten einde meer inligting oor die persoonlikheid en belangstellings van die individu te kan insluit. — die onderhoudsprosedure kan moontlik gerig word ten einde potensiele groei van die individu te probeer meet. laasgenoemde sal impliseer dat onderhoudsvoerders moet opgelei word in die kuns van onderhoudsvoering vir hierdie bepaalde doel dit is ook aan te beveel dat die prestasie op keuringsprosedure gekorreleer word met praktiese vaardighede van terapeute. aangesien keuring gerig is om studente te selekteer vir die kursus moet daar gewaak word teen die opskuif van standaarde ten einde 'n 'normale verspreidingskurwe' met betrekking tot punte te verkry, wat noodwendig dui op 'n groepie onsuksesvolle kandidate. laastens is die opleiding van personeel in die toesighoudende situasie van kardinale belang om te verseker dat die student die nodige leiding kry om te kan groei tot 'n volwaardige terapeut. bedankings graag word erkenning gegee aan die volgende: afdeling universiteitsonderwysbeplanning (studentediensburo, universiteit van pretoria) vir waardevolle inligting. mev f woodrow (kliniese sielkundige, department spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, up) vir inligting oor en evaluasie van sielkundige toetse. mev e.m louw (departement statistiek, up) vir hulp met statistiese verwerkings. verwysings cattell r.b., eber h.w. en tatsoeka m.m. handbook for the 16 personality factor questionnaire. illinois, institute for personality and ability testing, 1970. furler-kools s. de toelatingsprocedure. logopedie en foniatrie, 1981, 53, 11-13. hilson f. en mckendrick b. selecting students for admission to undergraduate courses of social work education and training. maatskaplike werk/social work, 1978, 14, 3, 108-114. lewis c. supervision in the training of speech therapists. unpublished dissertation submitted to the department of speech pathology and the witwatersrand, johannesburg, south africa, 1983. malan s.p.t. evaluering van die keuringstelsel waarvolgens voornemende kandidate vir die diploma in mondhigiene vanaf 1974-1978 gekeur is. ongepubliseerde stuk: studentediensburo, universiteit van pretoria, 1979. nuwe suid-afrikaanse groeptoets. raad vir geesteswetenskaplike navorsing, instituut vir psigometriese navorsing, 1965. oratio a.r. supervision in speech pathology. baltimore, university park press, 1977. senior aanlegtoets. raad vir geesteswetenskaplike navorsing, instituut vir psigometriese navorsing, 1975. j staf van de opleiding hoensbroek. opleiding logopedie, hoensbroek, 1981, 53, 24-38. ! steyn a.g.w., smit c.f. en du toit s.h.c. moderne statistiek vir die praktyk. pretoria, van schaik, 1984. | the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acoustiμ ε d hearing services a c o u s t i m e d (pty) l t d . , p.o. box 9 9 8 8 j o h a n n e s b u r g 2000 s o u t h a f r i c a t e l : ( 0 1 1 ) 3 3 7 2 9 7 7 p a n a v o x l ^ ^ danac rastronics hearing aids audiometers test equipment computer systems head office : 327 bosman buildings hoofkantoor : bosman gebou 327 cor. eloff and bree streets h/v eloff en bree strate • johannesburg ' 2001 johannesburg " 2001 june 1984 dear audiologist, one day soon you will sit behind the panel with the usual row of buttons and dials. of a new audiometer however, on this occasion you will press just one button and sit back while the audiometer runs through the tests. you will listen to a verbal report on the progress of the test while the audiogram is plotted on a video monitor (the patient will not realise that the test is being controlled by a machine). any inconsistencies in the patients responses will be checked and if things get really rough the audiometer will invite you to take over. thisis no gimmick or science fiction toy it is one of a /powerful new breed of instruments which use computer technology ' to provide consistent test procedures, data evaluation, statistical analysis, graphic storage etc. programmed entirely to your own .specifications by enhance · your procedures. j we provide full software backup and complete custom-designed computer systems.x please phone me for more details acoustimed these instruments •facilities for research, educational and clinical sincerely, d.a. smith managing director ' d i r e c t o r s / d i r e k t e u r e : d . a . s m i t h ( m a n a g i n g / b e s t u r e n d e ) , l . m . s m i t h die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 15 comorbidity of stuttering and disordered phonology in young children lesley wolk, ph.d (syracuse university) edward g. conture, ph.d (university of iowa) mary louise edwards, ph.d (stanford university) department of communication sciences and disorders syracuse university syracuse, new york, usa abstract young stutterers frequently exhibit concomitant speech and/or language disorders. the co-occurrence of these disorders is, however, not get well understood. the purpose of this paper is to introduce the notion of "comorbidity" as it relates to the field of speech-language pathology: specifically, to discuss comorbidity (coexistence) of stuttering and disordered phonology in young children. literature on concomitant speech and language disorders in young stutterers is reviewed, with special reference to the prevalence of articulatory/phonological disorders in young stutterers. future research on the coexistence of two speech and language disorders is encouraged, as well as the consideration of diagnostic treatment and prognostic implications for children who,exhibit both stuttering and disordered phonology as opposed to children who exhibit each disorder in isolation. opsomming jong hakkelaars vertoon dikwelssamegaandespraak en/of taalafivykings. diegelyktydige voorkoms van hierdie afivykings word tans egter nie ten voile bcgryp nie. die doel van hierdie artikel is om die begrip van "ko-morbiditeit" bekend te stel soos wat dit toegepas word op die vakgebied van spraakheelkunde en ook spesifiek om die gelyktydige voorkoms van hakkel en fonologiese afivykings in jong kinders te bespreek. 'n liter atuuroorsig van die gelyktydige voorkoms van spraak-en taalafivykings injong hakkelaars word verskaf met spesiale verwysing na die voorkoms van artikulasic/fonologiese afivykings injong hakkelaars. verdere navorsing oor die gelyktydige voorkoms van twee spraaken taalafivykings word aangemoedig. die oorweging van diagnostiese, behandelings en prognostiese implikasies vir kinders wat beide hakkel en afivykende fonologiese ontwikkeling vertoon. in teenstelling met kinders wat elke afwyking afionderlik vertoon. word aangebied. comorbidity refers to "... any distinct additional clinical entity that-has existed or that may occur during the clinical course of inpatient who has the index disease under study" (feinstein, 1970, p. 456). comorbidity has been discussed in some detail in the medical literature, particularly in relation to psychiatric disorders (boyd, burke, gruenberg, holzer, rae, george, karno, stoltzman, mcevoy &|nestadt, 1984; feinstein, 1970). yet it has received little attention in the field of speechlanguage pathology. although children with more than one speech disorder (e.g., stuttering and disordered phonology) are encountered frequently in clinical practice, there has been a paucity of research dedicated to understanding the coexistence and inter-relationships between two speech disorders. indeed, stuttering (s) and disordered phonology (dp) have traditionally been investigated and treated as two distinct disorders. little attempt has been made to merge the two disorders in terms of the following: (1) investigation of their cooccurrence in some children; (2) therapy regimens when both disorders are exhibited in the same child; and (3) conceptual explanations for their co-existence, and in some cases, persistence. the general purpose of this paper is to introduce the notion of "comorbidity" as it relates to the field of speech-language pathology. the more specific aim is to discuss the comorbidity (co-existence) of stuttering and disordered phonology exhibited in young children. literature on concomitant speech and die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif ir kommunikasieafwykins vol. 37 1990 language disorders in young stutterers is reviewed, with special reference to the prevalence of articulatory/phonological disorders in young stutterers. understanding the coexistence of two speech disorders in particular, stuttering (s) and disordered phonology (dp), has clinical implications, for example, differential diagnosis, such as the possibility of behavioral subgroups of young stutterers. further, diagnostic treatment and prognostic features may be different for children who exhibit the co-occurrence of two speech disorders as opposed to each disorder in isolation. feinstein (1970, p. 456) states: "with comorbidity omitted from consideration, two clinicians arguing about the merits of a mode of treatment for a particular disease may fail to recognize that their contradictory results arise not from the actions of treatment, but from the different associated diseases in the patients subjected to treatment." regarding the co-occurrence of stuttering and disordered phonology in young children, several studies have investigated the prevalence of their coexistence (e.g., blood & seider, 1981; daly, 1981). these studies are presented and discussed in detail below. in general, findings from previous studies have shown that 30-40% of young stutterers also exhibit articulation/phonological concerns (see table 1 below). however, only a few studies have attempted to explore the nature of this co-occurrence in more depth (e.g., st. louis & sasha 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 lesley wolk, edward g. conture and mary louise edwards hinzman, 1988; louko, conture & edwards, 1990). thus, little objective information is available regarding the nature and relation of these two disorders in young children. because it appears that approximately one third of children who stutter at one time or another exhibit articulation difficulties (e.g., cantwell & baker, 1985), it would seem important to increase our understanding of the nature and relation between stuttering and articulatory/phonological disorders in young children. to further highlight the existence and clinical importance of the co-occurrence of these two disorders, it is noted that clinicians frequently report that young children who are being treated for articulation difficulties may subsequently begin to stutter. comas (19 74, cited in bloodstein, 198 7, p. 221) reported that out of 1,050 cases of young children, in some, stuttering was observed to appear while they were being treated for articulation difficulties. in addition, with reference to childtable 1: published studies on the co-occurrence of stuttering and articulation/phonological difficulties in young children author date stut. ν nonstut. source of information % stut. with artie. diff. % nonstut. with artie. diff. summary of findings 1. mcdowell (1928) 33 33 speech exam. (articulation test) 19% 16% articulation difficulties with significant difference between groups 2. schindler (1955) 126 252 speech exam. 49% 15% "other speech disorders" 3. darley (1955) 50 50 parental reports 26% 4% associated articulation difficulties 4. morley (1957) 37 113 speech exam. 50% 31% "other speech disorders" 5. andrews and harris (1964) 77 78 parental reports 30% 10% associated articulation difficulties 6. williams and silverman (1968) 115 115 speech exam. 24% 9% .associated articulation difficulties 7. van riper (1971) 250-300 clinical records 14-25% delayed speech and language, articulation difficulties or evidence of organic involvement (track ii stutterers) 8. riley and riley (1979) 100 — speech exam. 33% associated articulation difficuties ! 9. preus (1981) 100 clinical records 18% van riper's track ii stutterers [ 10. daly (1981) 138 speech exam. 58% articulation j disorders ^ 11. blood and seider (1981) 1060 clinical reports 16% articulation difficulties 12. seider, gladstein and kidd (1982) 201 201 (siblings) parental reports — — no significant difference between groups 13. thompson (1983) 48 speech exam. 35-45% "suspected articulation difficulties" 14. cantwell and baker (1985) 40 speech exam 30% 15. st. louis and hinzman (1988) 48 24 speech exam. 67-96% / 16. louko, edwards and conture (1990) 30 30 speech exam. 40% 7% associated articulation difficulties the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comorbidity of stuttering and disordered phonology in young children 17 ren treated for language disorders, merits-patterson & reed (1981) recently showed that speech disfluencies can increase for some children who receive speech/language therapy. they investigated 27 preschool children classified into 3 groups of 9 each: language delayed children who had received language therapy, language delayed children.who had not received therapy, and those children with normal language development. none of the 27 children had ever been diagnosed as stutterers. they found that the group of language delayed children who received therapy produced significantly more whole-word and part-word repetitions (after therapy) than the other two groups. although there have been few published reports on the influence of therapy on other aspects of young stutterers' speech and language apart from the studies by comas (1974; cited in bloodstein, 1987), and merits-patterson & reed (1981), clinical reports suggest that stuttering often occurs secondary to the treatment of phonological and language disorders in young children; but, to our knowledge, the reverse has never been reported. c o n c o m i t a n t s p e e c h a n d l a n g u a g e d i s o r d e r s i n y o u n g s t u t t e r e r s p r e v a l e n c e o f a r t i c u l a t o r y / p h o n o l o g i c a l d i s o r d e r s in y o u n g s t u t t e r e r s a review of studies from 1928-1990 is presented in table 1. for each study, the author(s), date, sample size, source of information, percent stutterers and nonstutterers with articulation disorders, and major findings are summarized. "major findings" refers to the major characteristics pertaining to the stutterers for that study. the first of these studies was conducted by mcdowell (1928). he matched 33 stutterers and 33 nonstutterers according to age, sex, intelligence, native language and racial background. for both groups, the mean age was 10 years (range = 7-12 years). a nonstandardized articulation test was used, in which each child was required to repeat a series of sentences after an examiner, who recorded errors in the production of vowels, diphthongs, consonants and consonant clusters. findings indicated that the mean error rates for the stutterers and nonstutterers were 19% and 16%,'respectively. this represented a small but statistically significant difference between the two groups. mcdowell questioned the validity of these findings, however, because subjective scoring procedures were employed. moreover, it could be argued that repetition of sounds in sentences is a different form of speech elicitation than a naming task or conversational speech, since an imitation task may overestimate the child's performance. subsequent studies in the 1950's made reference to the presence of "other speech disorders" in young stutterers, with only vague suggestion that these "other disorders" were most likely to be articulation difficulties (morley, 1957; schindler, 1955). for example, schindler (1955) found that 49% of 126 stuttering children had "other" speech disorders, whilst this was evident in only 15% of 252 nonstutterers. similarly, morley (1957) reported that 50% of 37young stutterers and 31 % of 113 nonstutterers had "other speech disorders". it is difficult to determine from these early studies exactly what was implied by "other speech disorders". however, it is assumed that many of these were difficulties with speech sound production. die huui-ajrikuunse 'lytlskril vir kornmumkasicafwtikiiifp, vol. 37. i!).')!) more recent studies have reported specifically on the prevalence of articulation difficulties in young stutterers. williams & silverman (1968) found 24% of 115 school-aged stutterers had associated articulation difficulties. riley & riley (1979) showed this to be the case in 33% of 100 young stutterers. daly (1981) reported that 58% of a subgroup of 25 young stutterers (η = 25), out of a larger sample (n = 138), exhibited articulation disorders. thompson (1983) observed a 35-45% prevalence of suspected articulation difficulties in two samples (n = 3 1 & n = 1 7 ) o f young stutterers. recently, cantwell & baker (1985) reported a prevalence of approximately 30% in a sample of 40 young stutterers out of a larger sample of 600 children with speech and/or language disorders. st. louis & hinzman (1988) found that 67-96% of their school-aged stutterers (n = 48) had articulation difficulties. in general, they found that young stutterers are likely to manifest other communicative impairments, especially in articulation and voice. further, several studies have indicated a prevalence of 15-30% articulation difficulties in young stutterers based on clinical and/or parental reports (e.g., andrews & harris, 1964; darley, 1955). van riper (1971), using clinical records and related observations, reported that 14-25% of young stutterers had "de-layed speech and language, articulation difficulties or evidence of organic involvement". he categorized these as "track ii" stutterers. preus (1981) studied the clinical records of 100 young norwegian stutterers, and reported that 18% had similar difficulties and could be classified as van riper's "track ii" stutterers. blood & seider (1981) found that, among caseload reports of 1,060 young stutterers, 16% exhibited articulation difficulties. however, it is difficult to interpret this result meaningfully because blood & seider did not employ a control group of nonstutterers. furthermore, the criteria used in diagnosing articulation difficulties varied among clinicians (cf. nippold, 1990). most recently, louko. edwards & conture (1990) found that among 30 stutterers and 30 age-matched nonstutterers, 40% of the stutterers exhibited articulation difficulties as opposed to 7% of the nonstutterers. one study that did not support the view that stutterers have a higher incidence of articulation disorders than nonstutterers is that by seider, gladstein & kidd (1982). in their study, informants were questioned about the presence of articulation disorders in stutterers and same-sex nonstuttering siblings. results showed that stutterers and nonstutterer siblings did not differ significantly in the frequency of associated articulation difficulties. instead, articulation difficulties occurred most frequently in late talking subjects compared to early (or average) speakers regardless of the presence or absence of stuttering. findings of this study suggested that language and articulation onset and development may be more a function of familial patterns and gender than of stuttering. in general, more studies support than refute the finding that articulation disorders frequently co-exist with stuttering in young children. however, an important consideration in reviewing these studies is the variation in assessment methodology. that is, some studies have used direct examination/observation of children's speech production, whereas others have relied on questionnaire data and/or parental reports. this one in addition to other methodological considerations has been highlighted in a recent critique of the literature on concomitant speech and language disorders in stuttering children (nippold, 1990). these are: r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 lesley wolk, edward g. conture and mary louise edwards (a) the use of parental interview or informal observation in place of direct testing of children (e.g., andrews & harris, 1964; darley, 1955; seider et al. 1982). (b) the absence of data establishing test-retest and interscorer reliability of articulation assessment (e.g., blood & seider, 1981; mcdowell, 1928; williams & silverman, 1968). (c) the difficulty in distinguishing true articulation errors from manifestations of stuttering (e.g., schindler, 1955). (d) the absence of ethnic and linguistic background matching criteria. a recent study (wolk, 1990) was designed to overcome some of these methodological concerns, in an attempt to further explore the co-occurrence of s+dp in young children. wolk compared the behaviours of children who exhibited both s+dp with those of children who exhibited each disorder in isolation. the methods employed were (a) use of the 162-item picture naming task for direct testing of children's speech articulation, (b) intraand inter-rater reliability measures and (c) clearly developed criteria for distinguishing between true articulation errors and stutterings. findings from this study suggest that stutterers with phonological concerns exhibit some unique disfluency characteristics (e.g., significantly more sound prolongations) which distinguish them from stutterers without phonological difficulties. there are also reports of "language delay" in young children who stutter, although this does not appear to be nearly as prevalent in young stutterers as articulation difficulties (bloodstein, 1987). furthermore, it is often difficult to determine from these studies whether language delay refers exclusively to syntactic, semantic and/or cognitive factors, or whether it is a more global term including phonological difficulties. the following section provides and overview of studies on language delay in young stutterers. l a n g u a g e d e l a y s i n c h i l d r e n w h o s t u t t e r some investigators have reported that stutterers tend to be slow in developing language (berry, 1938; morley, 1957), although the iowa studies (of nearly 200 stutterers and their matched controls) showed slight or no differences (bloodstein, 1987). andrews & harris (1946, p. 35) speculated that the population groups used as subjects in the iowa studies tended to be representative of higher socio-economic levels which could possibly explain the difference between their findings and those of other studies. more recently, accordi et al. (1983, cited in bloodstein, 1987, p. 215) found "...retarded language development" in 28 percent of stutterers as opposed to 8.7 percent of a control group. conversely, bernstein ratner & costa sih's (1987) results do not support subtle language differences between normal and stuttering children. however, their findings suggest that disfluency breakdown is significantly correlated with gradual increases in syntactic complexity for both stuttering and normalchildren. some studies have investigated the co-occurrence of disfluency with specific syntactic structures in 2-4 year old normally developing children (colburn & mysak, 1988a, 1982b; helmreich & bloodstein, 1973). helmreich & bloodstein found that pronouns and conjunctions appeared in significantly greater proportion among the disfluent words, than did nouns and verbs. colburn & mysak concluded that "developmental disfluency was more strongly attached to the syntax of utterances than to the production of particular words" (1982b, p. 421). further, they concluded that"... the cognitive effort exerted in learning syntactic structures is reflected in systematic changes in speech disfluency in the early languagelearning period" (p. 425).' murray & reed (1977j reported that preschool stutterers scored significantly lower than their controls on the peabody picture vocabulary test (ppvt), the northwestern syntax screening test (nsst), and the verbal abilities scale of the zimmerman preschool language scale. kline & starkweather (1979) found that stutterers (aged 3:0 to 6:0 years) had a significantly lower mean length of utterance (m.l.u.) than did nonstutterers, as well as lower scores on the carrow test for auditory comprehension of language. in further support for a language delay, westby (1979) showed that her stutterers scored significantly poorer than normal speaking children in regard to frequency of grammatical errors, in receptive vocabulary on the ppvt, and in responses on semantic tasks selected from the torrance test of creative thinking. in a syntactic analysis of the speech of four stutterers (aged 5:0 to 6:0 years) and four age-matched controls, wall (1980) found that the stutterers tended to use simpler, less mature language. conversely, meyers & freeman (1985) reported no significant differences in m.l.u. between 4:0 to 5:0 year old stutterers and their nonstuttering peers during communicative interaction with their mothers. most recently, enger, hood & shulman (1988) examined both language and fluency characteristics of 20 linguistically advanced preschool and school-aged children (aged 3:2 to 7:0 years). they found that, although these linguistically-advanced children exhibited slightly more frequent disfluencies than would be expected, their disfluency patterns paralleled those characteristics of normal speakers (i.e., interjections and revisions). the majority of their disfluencies were "semantically more filled than empty," occurred internal (rather than external) to the constituent clause, and appeared to be neither physically tense nor highly fragmented. i thus to date, only limited data are available to support language differences in stutterers, with research results being equivocal regarding the prevalence and specific nature of language abilities between stutterers and nonstutterers. ! some possible explanations for young stutterers' concomitant speech and ' language problems i i there have been very few speculations about the meaning of young stutterers' concomitant speech and language problems. furthermore, few of these speculations have been supported with empirical research. one view, which takes a psychosocial perspective, is that held by bloodstein (1975, cited in bloodstein, 1987). he suggested that children with communication disorders are more likely to acquire a sense of failure as speakers and thus learn to struggle with their speech attempts. a second view is that there is a common predisposition underlying the twoproblems (stuttering and other speech and/or language problems); that is, they are caused by some ex tent.-by the same thing (bloodstein, 1987, p. 221). for example, west, kennedy & carr (1947, p. 93) suggested that "stuttering" and "speech retardath south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comorbidity of stuttering and disordered phonology in young children 19 tion" often tend to appear in the same individuals because they have inherited a common predisposition to both conditions. a third view is perhaps a subcategory of the second view, in that both stuttering and associated speech/language problems are speculated to be caused by the same phenomenon; specifically a "central neurological processing deficit" (byrd & cooper, 1988). there is some preliminary support for this speculation via empirical research, which is discussed below. byrd & cooper (1988) administered the blakeley screening test for developmental apraxia of speech (stdas) to 16 young stutterers, 15 d e v e l o p m e n t a l ^ apraxic children, and 15 normal speaking children aged.4:0 to 9:0 years. results indicated that although significant differences were observed among the three groups on the overall test score (8 subtests), the apraxic and stuttering groups performed similarly on all stdas subtests except for the articulation subtest. specifically, they interpreted their findings to provide support for a possible "central neurological processing deficit" in some young stutterers. also, observations by yoss & darley (1974) suggest that in some children, articulatory problems and stuttering might both be manifestations of "developmental apraxia". among 30 children with articulation problems, sixteen performed poorly on a test of oral apraxia. in addition, these children had more repetitions and prolongations in their speech than did the others. there is still some controversy, however, as to the precise definition of the term "developmental apraxia", and, in fact, as to the existence of this disorder as a clinical entity. concluding remarks in conclusion, bloodstein (1987) recently stated: "there is hardly a finding more thoroughly confirmed in the whole range of comparative studies of stutterers and nonstutterers than the tendency of stutterers to have functional difficulties of articulation, 'immature' speech and the like" (p. 219-220). it seems, then, that the approximately 30-40% prevalence of articulation difficulties in young stutterers is greater than the approximate 2-6.4% prevalence that would be expected in a typical population (beitchman, nair.clegg & patel, 1986; hull, mielke, timmons & williford, 1971). thus, articulation disorders appear to be one of the speech-language disorders most commonly associated path stuttering. although much literature isjavailable regarding the nature of speech disfluencies in young stutterers and the nature of phonological difficulties in young children, there is still limited information regarding the co-occurrence of the two disorders in young children. investigation of this co-occurrence is encouraged since it would appear to have intrinsic value for a deeper understanding of each disorder separately, as well as for the relationship between the two disorders. in addition, we believe such research may have important clinical implications for treating these two coexisting speech disorders. it is hoped that this review will stimulate research and interest in comorbidity in speech-language pathology, in particular, in the interrelations between stuttering and disordered phonology and/or language delay in young children. finally, clinicians are urged to give specialized consideration to the diagnostic, treatment and prognostic implications for children who exhibit both stuttering and disordered phonology as opposed to those who exhibit each disorder in isolation. acknowledgements supports in part by osep grants (6000850252 & h023c8008) to syracuse university, syracuse, new york. thanks to the human sciences research council (hsrc), pretoria, south africa, for financial assistance to the first author. references andrews, g„ & harris, m. the syndrome ofstutteriny, chapters 4 , 5 , 6 . london: the spastics society medical education and information unit in association with williams heineman medical books, 1964. beitchman, j.h., nair, r„ clegg, m. &, patel, p.c. prevalence of speech and language disorders in 5-year-old kindergarten children in the ottawa-carleton region journal of speech and heariny disorders, 51, 98-110, 1986. bernstein ratner, n. & costa sin, c. effects of gradual increases m sentence length and complexity in children's disfluency.jowrnal of speech and heariny disorders, 52, 278-287, 1987. berry, m.f. development history of stuttering children. journal of pediatrics, 12, 209-217,1938. blood, g.w. & seider, r. the concomitant problems of young stutterers. journal of speech and heariny disorders, 46, 31-33, 1981. bloodstein, o. a handbook on stutteriny (4th ed.). chicago, 1l.: national easter seal society, 1987. boyd, j.h., burkej.d., gruenberg, e„ holzer, c.e., rae, d.s., george, l.k., karno, m., stoltzman, r., mcevoy, l. & nestadt, g. exclusion criteria of dsm-i11: a study of co-occurrence of hierarchyfree syndromes. archives of general psychiatry, 41, 983-990, 1984. byrd, k. & cooper, e.b. apraxic speech characteristics in stutteriny, developmentally apraxic and normally speaking children. paper presented at the annual convention of the american speech-language-hearing association (asha), boston, ma., 1988 (november). cantwell, d. & baker, l. psychiatric and learning disorders in children with speech and language disorders: a descriptive analysis. advances in learning and behavioral disabilities, 2, 29-47,1985. colburn, n. & mysak, e.d. developmental disfluency and emerging grammar 1. disfluency characteristics in early syntactic utterances. journal of speech and heariny research, 25, 414-420, 1982a. colburn, n. & mysak, e.d. developmental disfluency and emerging grammer ii. co-occurrence of disfluency with specified semantic-syntactic structures. jo»r«fli of speech and heariny research, 25, 421-427, 1982b. daly, d. differentiation of stuttering subgroups with van riper's developmental tracks: a preliminary study .journal of the national student speech lanyuaye heariny association, 9, 89-101, 1981. darley, f. the relationship of parental attitudes and adjustments to the development of stuttering. in w.johnson and r. leutenegger (eds.), stutteriny in children and adults. minneapolis. mn: university of minnesota press, 1955. enger, n.c., hood, s.b., & shulman, b.b. language and fluency variables in the conversational speech of linguistically advanced preschool and school-aged children. journal of fluency disorders, 13, 173-198, 1988. feinstein, a.r. the pre-therapeutic classification of co-morbidity in chronic disease journal of chronic diseases, 23, 455-468, 1970. helmreich, h.g. & bloodstein, o. the grammatical factor in childhood disfluency in relation to the continuity hypothesis. jou rnal of speech and heariny research. 16, 731-738, 1973. hull, f.m., mielke, p.w., timmons, r.j. & willeford, j.a. the national speech and hearing survey: preliminary results. asha, 13, 501-509, 1971. kline, m.l. & starkweather, c.w. perceptive and expressive language performance in young stutterers. asha, 21, 797, abstract, 1979. louko, l.j., edwards, m.l. & conture, e.g. phonological characteristics of ifouny stutterers. manuscript submitted for publication, 1990. mcdowell e.d. educational and emotional adjustment ofstutteriny children. new york: columbia university teachers college, 1928. merits-patterson, r. & reed, c.g. disfluencies in the speech oflanguage-delayed children .journal ofspeech and heariny research, 46. 55-58, 1981. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykinys, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 lesley wolk, edward g. conture and mary louise edwards meyers, s.c. & freeman, f.j. are mothers of stutterers different? an investigation of social-communicative interaction. journal of fluency disorders, 10, 193-209, 1985. marley, m.e. the development of disorders of speech in childhood. edinburgh, scotland: livingstone, 1957. murray, h.l. & reed, c.c. language abilities of preschool stuttering children. journal of fluency disorders, 2, 171-176, 1977. nippold, m.a. concomitant speech and language disorders in stuttering children: a critique of the literature. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 55,51-60, 1990. preus, a. attempts at identifying subgroups of stutterers. oslo, norway: university of oslo press, 1981. riley, g.d. & riley, j. a component model for diagnosing and treating children who stutter .journal of fluency disorders, 4 279-293 1979. st. louis, k.o. & hinzman, a.r. a descriptive study of speech, language and hearing characteristics of school-aged stutterers. journal of fluency disorders. 13, 331-355, 1988. schindler, m. a study of educational adjustments of stuttering and nonstuttering children. in w.johnson & r. leutenegger (eds.), stuttering in children and adults. minneapolis, mn: university of minnesota press, 1955. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 37, 1990 seider, r.a., gladstein, k.l. & kidd, k.k. language onset and concomitant speech and language problems in subgroups of stutterers and their s i b l i n g s . o f speech and hearinq research, 25, 482-486, 1982. thompson, j. assessment of fluency in school-age children. resource guide. danville, 1l: interstate printers and publishers, 1983. van riper, c. the nature of stutterinq. englewood cliffs, nj: prentice hall, 1971. wall, m.j. acomparison of syntax inyoung stutterers and nonstutters. journal of fluency disorders, 12, 133-145, 1980. west, r„ kennedy, l. & carr, a. the rehalnlitation of speech (rev. ed.). new york: harper bros, 1947. westby, c.e. language performance of stuttering and nonstuttering children. j«/ n k u ; f , 9 e r s t " l d r pienaar .e,f dat hy horn ge,oepe g e v t , \ . t — ^ j ^ t a l s universiteit van pretoria te vestig hy het reeds.η i ho.;'g.word v a , = departement s p r . a ^ w e t e n s k a p e n s p r a a k h e e i k u i n a , a f e e s t u d e e r by uuste^spraakwetenskaplike, ^ ^ ^ k g g k h f d stelling en uitgebreide kennis oor die, toule vakgeb het gedurig tot vernuwing gelei. geen wonder , η 27 aoril 1993 in d i e senaateaal, u n i v e r s i t e i t v a n p r e t o r i a . * hierdie artikel is as professorale intreerede gelewer op 27 april die suid-afrikaanse tydskf vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 isabel c. uys met die kennis-ontploffing gedurende die 1960's gesorg het dat die naam van die departement verander na die departement spraakwetenskap, spraakheelkunde en oudiologie nie. dit is egter so dat ontwikkeling van 'n vakgebied r a s i o n a l i s a s i e n o o d s a a k u i t b r e i d i n g , sowel as inperking. in 1983, met die afskaffing van die vak spraakwetenskap vir graaddoeleindes, is die naam van die d e p a r t e m e n t v e r a n d e r na d i e d e p a r t e m e n t spraakheelkunde en oudiologie. vooruitgang op die gebied van taalteorie-ontwikkeling en die implikasies daarvan op taalafwykings het egter weer toevoeging noodsaaklik gemaak. spraak-taalpatologie en oudiologie, en spraak-taal-gehoorterapie het reeds wereldwyd algemene en aanvaarde vakterme geword. ongelukkig is dit so dat, as gevolg van die konseptuele verskille, geen een van hierdie begrippe uitgelaat kan word sonder om afbreuk te doen aan die omvang van menslike kommunikasie en dus ook die naam van die departement nie. dit laat ons met 'n dilemma van lompheid en fragmentasie in die benaming wat aangespreek moet word. deur al die j a r e is prof p i e n a a r se filosofiese uitgangspunt weerspieel in die missie van die departement om 'n holistiese en organismiese benadering tot kommunikasiepatologie te bevorder en om beroepsbeoefening as 'n idealisties-gei'nspireerde roeping te beskou. maar, die uitbreiding van die veld en die toevoeging van kennis het noodwendig tot spesialisasie en f r a g m e n t a s i e g e l e i . steeds is dit die e n i g s t e omskrewe b e r o e p wat dienslewering aan kommunikasiegestremdes ten doel het die beroep wat die diagnose, habilitasie en rehabilitasie van diegene met kommunikasieafwykings (spraak-, taalen gehoorafwykings) omvat. selfevaluasies die weegskaal vir onderrig en beroep uit die voorafgaande blyk dit duidelik dat daar aan die een kant volgehoue vordering op die vakgebied en in die beroep voorkom, maar aan die ander kant moet die toepaslikheid en effektiwiteit van onderrig en dienslewering in andersoortige en veranderende omstandighede g e d u r i g b e v r a a g t e k e n w o r d . n o o i t moet verandering gelykgestel word aan onstabiliteit nie. die afwesigheid van verandering lei nie tot stabiliteit nie, maar tot stagnasie. mens moet dus altyd ingestel wees op daardie tekens wat 'n behoefte aan verandering en 'n geleentheid vir vordering aandui (feldman, 1981, p.942). 'n behoefte aan kritiese evaluasie reflekteer dus nie noodwendig onsekerheid en ontevredenheid nie, maar eerder 'n bewustelike, sensitiewe ingesteldheid om te weeg en nooit onverhoeds te lig bevind te word nie. in 1966 het prof pienaar gese: "...one does feel that a substantial part of the idealistic program which was planned in 1936 has already been realized. a young country, with a comparatively small percentage of wage earners, keen on expansion in every sphere of life, with no endowments and handicapped by a lack of funds, has had to march foreward on faith, hope and charity and its youthful idealism. we are jealous of our standards of training, of research and therapeutics. we are eager to learn from those who can spare more manpower for research. we realize the vastness of the field still lying fallow; the great task still ahead of us to cater to the needs of the whole population of south africa and through south africa for the whole of the awakening southern africa." (rieber & brubaker, 1966, p. 600-601). as the development of the profession was initially based on models from europe (especially germany with its strong physiologic orientation) and america (the founders of behaviourism in our field), the training and service delivery in this country is still geared towards the western model. i am not saying "...that a foreign model modified on the basis of limited information is necessarily inadequate, but rather that it is important that we be aware that this is the situation." (delaney & malan, 1984, p. 75). and where has this adherence to the western model led us? i quote from the main report on disability in the republic of south africa (1987, p. 14): "...it is clear that the existing services are distributed unevenly in the development regions. there are in fact development regions which completely lack certain basic services for the disabled. it is further noticed that in many respects there is an unequal distribution of services among the various population groups... the complete view that is obtained at the macrolevel is that services are mainly limited to the metropolitan areas and that a significant percentage of disabled people have little or no access to the existing services. it is, therefore, selfevident that in a programme for the upgrading of services, the horisontal distribution of services must be examined before generally vertical extension of services can be considered." even before the investigation into disability in south africa, the south african speech-language-hearing association took steps based on the realisation that three issues should be addressed: " the multifaceted nature of the linguistic and socio-cultural make-up of various communities in south africa; the consequences of this on the effective implementation of tasks performed by speech and hearing therapists, and the extreme manpower shortage of speech and hearing therapists in the country." (aron, 1984, p. 1). x these service delivery issues reflect on training within the southern african context. we realise that "we can either shape our own future, or we can live with a profession someone else shapes" (cole, 1986, p. 41). external evaluation had already been initiated by t h e u n i n i t i a t e d in t h e f i e l d , and u n f o r t u n a t e l y the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) kommunikasiepatologie: onderrig vir die toekoms misconstruction of the facts can harm, not only the profession, but also the clients we serve. it was time for the profession to shape its own future. extensive and in-depth self-evaluation was indeed called for, taking cognizance of the stated scope of our profession, weighing it in terms of the needs of the population of disabled, the needs of the professionals, the existing resources, the infrastructure and the demands made by the system. it was reasonably easy to identify the scope of the profession as it is internationally acknowledged that a speech-language pathologist specialises in the diagnosis and treatment of speech and language problems, and engages in the scientific study of human communication. diagnoses are made of speech and language competencies of individuals, including the assessment of speech and language skills as related to educational, medical, social and psychological factors. human communicative efficiency of individuals with communication problems of organic or nonorganic origin is restored through planning, directing or conducting habilitative or rehabilitative treatment programmes. counselling and guidance to speech and language handicapped individuals, as well as consultation with educational, medical and other professionals, are provided. the scientific principles of human communication are taught. projects investigating epidemiological and biosocial phenomena, associated with speech, voice and language are directed and research is conducted to develop, design and evaluate diagnostic and remedial techniques and apparatus. but, as in south africa professionals have a double qualification, the scope of the audiologist had to be included, which states that the audiologist specialises in the diagnostic evaluation of hearing, prevention of hearing problems, habilitative and rehabilitative services for individuals with auditory problems. electroacoustic instrumentation is used to determine the range, nature and degree of hearing function related to the patient's auditory efficiency (his communication needs). audiometric results are coordinated with other diagnostic data, such as educational, medical, social and behavioural information. conservation, habilitative and rehabilitative programmes are planned, directed and conducted. teaching and research in the physiology, pathology, biophysics and psychophysics of the auditory system are carried out. consultation with educational, medical and other professional groups is provided (flower, 1984, p. 5). in considering these definitions of the scope of the profession, we realised that due to the complex nature of human communication and its disorders, it is essential that information of an academic, research and scientific nature is continually developed and evaluated. a university provides the optimal environment for constant academic validation of current professional training as teaching in the field of speech-language pathology and audiology is directed towards the accumulation and integration of theoretical knowledge rather than technical skills. it was, however, more difficult to identify the demands, threats and opportunities that we are faced with in the present south african context. during 1989-1990 our department undertook an investigation (uys & hugo, 1989) aimed at a situational 5 and needs analysis. information, based on questionnaire responses and personal interviews, gathered from a sample of nearly 1000 people throughout south africa, was utilised in the formulation of a vision of the future in clem sunter's words the story of our profession and training. as the story unfolded, we realised that our profession, and indeed our training, had reached the proverbial crossroads. first of all we had to sell our story to all those involved in training and service delivery and, as is the case with all salesmen in economically straitened circumstances, this was, and to a certain extent still is, an unenviable and painful task. it is never easy to venture into the unknown, but as in goal-directed and purposeful selling one has to teach the buyer to take off his own shoes before he can step into another person's (johnson & wilson, 1987). the difficulty did not lie in the new ideas, but in the ability and willingness to escape from the old ones. in this case to renounce the often impractical and irrelevant, for models and strategies which promise to be appropriate and applicable within the southern african context (crossley, 1986). we then had to develop strategies for the implementation of action plans to test the validity and viability of our new story. this i would like to share with you. kruispaaie in onderrig en dienslewering opleiding is die een enkel grondslag waarop effektiewe, kwaliteit dienslewering gebaseer word. maar omdat hierdie opleiding afstuur op sowel 'n akademiese as 'n professionele kwalifikasie is dit nodig om te voldoen aan akademies-wetenskaplike eise, navorsingseise en beroepseise. die tyd het aangebreek om die opleiding te weeg teen hierdie eise. is hierdie vakgebied noodwendig 'n wetenskaplikgefundeerde dissipline? is universiteitsopleiding werklik nodig? is spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe nie maar net resepopmakers, waarvoor 'n laervlak tegniese opleiding voldoende en heelwat goedkoper sal wees nie? wat is die minimum vereistes wat aan onderrig en dienslewering in suid-afrika gestel moet word? voldoen die bestaande opleiding aan al die gestelde eise? wat is die realiteit? die rsa is hoofsaaklik 'n derdewereldland, met eiesoortige probleme, beperkings, behoeftes en uitdagings, waarin die beroep oorwegend as 'n luuksheid gesien word. in 'n ontwikkelende land word die spraak-taalterapeut en oudioloog met e i e s o o r t i g e probleme gekonfronteer en dienslewering (en gevolglik ook opleiding) moet daarby aangepas word. die relevansie van die tradisionele opleiding is dus bevraagteken. die rsa beleef 'n gevaarlike bevolkingsontploffing. in 1984 was daar na raming reeds 'n tekort van 4 4y4 spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe in die rsa. in 1986 het die tekort gestyg tot oor die 5 000 en volgens die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 isabel c. uys projeksie sal daar in die jaar 2000 'n tekort van minstens 10 000 wees. die rsa het 'n unieke multikulturele, veeltalige bevolkingsamestelling. dit het aan die lig gekom dat bykans alle dienste u i t s l u i t l i k gerig is op bevoorregte, stedelike blankes. dienste aan die ontwikkelende plattelandse bevolkingsgroepe is totaal ontoereikend en voorkomingsdienste is feitlik nerens beskikbaar nie. by verreweg die meeste terapeute is slegs afrikaans of engels magtig, terwyl die grootste behoefte by anderstaliges gevind word. in die geval van spraak-, taalen gehoorterapie is taal en kommunikasie so uiters belangrik, omdat dit beide die middel en die doel van intervensie is. die opleiding en dienslewering van spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe geskied steeds binne die raamwerk van eerstewereldse, westerse modelle. gesofistikeerde hoe-tegnologie word in die meeste gevalle in gei'nstitusionaliseerde praktyke aangewend. diens in die gemeenskap aan die gemeenskap bestaan omtrent nie. die standaard van opleiding en diens wat gelewer word, word internasionaal as uitnemend beskou. spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe wat hier kwalifiseer bereik groot sukses met nagraadse studies en navorsing oorsee. hulle voldoen aan die eise van kliniese vaardigheidsprofiele oorsee. hulle word met ope arms in die internasionale arbeidsmag ontvang. namate al hierdie feite aan die lig gekom het, het daar nog 'n paar vraagtekens ontstaan. indien die opleiding en dienslewering dan van so 'n hoe gehalte is, waarom verkies van ons beste graduandi om oorsee te gaan werk? kan dit dalk toegeskryf word aan 'n onvermoe om beroepsbevrediging te verkry, juis omdat terapeute nie opgelei word vir die tipe praktyk wat hulle plaaslik beoefen nie? waarom is daar steeds so 'n tekort aan beskikbare, bereikbare, gelykwaardige diens aan alle taalen k u l t u u r g r o e p e in die land? die enigste gevolgtrekking wat gemaak kan word, is dat daar erens 'n wanpassing is tussen die aard van die opvoeding en dienslewering aan die een kant, en die aard van die behoeftes en eise van die gestremde bevolking aan die ander kant. hierdie hipotese is bewys deur antwoorde op die vraelyste wat van b e r o e p s l u i , die professionele vereniging, die beroepsraad, universiteitspersoneel en werkgewers ontvang is: die algemene indruk is dat daar in die opleiding 'n hoe standaard gehandhaaf word. die studie van menslike kommunikasie behels 'n sterk filosofiese en universeel-wetenskaplike onderbou basiese wetenskaplike en universeel-teoretiese vakinhoud, omdat die hantering van die kommunikasieafwykende afhanklik is van 'n grondige kennis van al die prosesse o n d e r l i g g e n d aan n o r m a l e menslike kommunikasie. juis as gevolg hiervan is dit die tipe opvoeding wat steeds op universiteitsvlak aangebied behoort te word. daar is orals 'n mannekragtekort, sodat terapeute met onhanteerbare waglyste gekonfronteer word. meer spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe is nodig; 'n herorientasie in die benadering tot beroepsfunksies is nodig; andersoortige beroepsbeoefenaars is nodig. in die bestaande kurrikulums word egter 'n gefragmenteerde, afwykinggerigte benadering beklemtoon. dit kan waarskynlik as toepaslik in 'n ryk eerstewereldse land beskou word, maar in die rsa-konteks sal fragmentasie en spesialisasie tot oneffektiewe d i e n s l e w e r i n g lei. 'n h o l i s t i e s e , funksionele benadering moet gevolg word, waar elke afwyking as 'n verbreking in die totale menslike kommunikasieproses, in perspektief geplaas moet word. spraak, taal en gehoor is slegs die waarneembare elemente van 'n omvattende kommunikasieafwyking, waarin die geheel belangriker as die som van die dele is. die beroepsomstandighede waarvoor opgelei word, is geleidelik besig om te verskuif vanaf die normale skoolsituasie na spesiale onderwys, die hospitaal en veral die privaat praktyk. deregulering in gesondheidsdienste is 'n werklikheid en ook terapeute neig al hoe meer om werkgewers te word en nie werknemers te bly nie. opvoeding moet dus voorsiening maak vir inskerping in andersoortige en meer omvattende beroepsfunksies, wat sal aanpas by die eise wat aan 'n werkvoorsiener gestel word. die bestaande westerse klem moet afrikagerig word. afrikakultuur en taal moet 'n integrate deel van die opleiding vorm. daar is 'n groot behoefte aan voortgesette onderrig, juis om die brug te slaan tussen westerse, hoe-tegnologieopleiding en afrikakonteks behoeftes, maar ook om volgehoue kwaliteit dienslewering in die toekoms te verseker. hierdie onderrig moet as gevolg van die mannekragtekort en geografiese verspreiding van beroepsbeoefenaars, deur afstandsonderrig aangebied word. wat die eise van professionele opleiding betref, was dit duidelik dat die volgende beginsels moet geld: die doelwitte van opvoeding moet gespesifiseer word in terme van die hele omvang van toepaslike bevoegdhede: kennis, vaardigheid en gesindheid. die leerproses wat tot hierdie bevoegdhede lei, vereis 'n kombinasie van akademiese en praktiese opleiding (cunnington, 1985, p. 76). wat die standaarde betref, moet die opleiding voldoen aan die eise van die universiteitswese, sowel as die minimum vereistes wat deur die beroepsraad vir spraak-taalterapie en oudiologie gestel word. dit was duidelik dat daar 'n nuwe, toepaslike model vir opleiding en dienslewering geskep moes word. hierdie model (uys & hugo, 1990) hou 'n veelvlak diensleweringshierargie voor, waarin verskillende soorte insette in 'n verskeidenheid van omstandighede aan die behoeftes van die kommunikasiegestremde in sy gemeenskap kan voldoen. kommunikasiegestremdheid moet aangespreek word vanaf die vlak van/primere gesondheidsorg, reg deur die spektrum van gesondheidsen rehabilitasiedienste, tot by die hoogste vlak van spesialisasie en tegnologiese ontwikkeling. uit die aard van die saak hou hierdie model dus ook implikasies vir opleiding in. tans is die universiteite die enigste opleidingsinstansies waar kursusse in kommunikasiepatologie aangebied word. voorgestelde the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) kommunikasiepatologie: onderrig vir die toekoms aanpassings en byvoeging van opleidingsprogramme moet dus aanvanklik die verantwoordelikheid van die universiteite se deskundiges wees. onderrig vir die toekoms: die verantwoordelikheid van die departement die departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie wil homself verbind tot verantwoordbare opleiding van studente vir die toekoms. met hierdie diensleweringsmodel as uitgangspunt, glo ek, is dit moontlik. op die makrovlak is die belangrikste faktor waaraan aandag geskenk word die voorsiening van spraak-, taalen gehoordienste, waarin op die regieplek, die regie diens deur en aan die regie mense gelewer sal word. hierdie stelling impliseer dat 1. genoeg persone verskillende soorte opleiding moet ontvang om 'n veelvlakdiens te lewer; 2. dienste in 'n groot mate gede'institusionaliseer moet word; 3. effektiewe diens op elke vlak gelewer moet word, veral met die oog op bestuur, supervisie en medekonsultasie; 4. persone kennis moet dra van die taal en kultuur van die gemeenskap van die gestremdes; 5. personeel in staat moet wees om deur opvoeding en verryking gemeenskappe te bekragtig om self te besluit oor hulle eie behoeftes ten opsigte van gestremdheid en intervensie. dit is vandag algemeen bekend dat gesondheidsberoepe gekritiseer word omdat hulle te hooggekwalifiseer is. is 'n minimum van 'n duur, vierjarige universiteitsgraad werklik nodig vir 'n persoon om basiese hulp te verleen aan duisende kommunikasiegestremdes wat tans geen hulp ontvang nie? daarteenoor, is dit eties verdedigbaar om komplekse menslike dienslewering te onderneem, sonder dat jy daarvoor opgelei is? kan die onopgeleide p|ersoon skade doen, of is 'n halwe eier beter as 'n lee dop? omdat daar baie bewyse is dat persone wat voorgee dat hulle opgelei is, wel meer kwaad as goed aan die kommunikasiegestremde !doen, kan minderwaardige; vinnige, goedkoop opleiding, net om gou meer werkers in die veld te kry, nie toegelaat word nie. maar 'n middeweg is tog moontlik deur werksverspreiding oor verskillende vlakke van dienslewering en samewerking. ek is geen voorstander van laer standaarde in opvoeding en dienslewering nie. daarom wil ek pleit vir die behoud van die hoogste standaarde op verskillende vlakke van opvoeding en dienslewering, waar persone in verskillende beroepskategoriee die beste opleiding ontvang om uitnemende diens op spesifieke vlakke te lewer. ι spraak-taalterapie en oudiologie is in 'ri groot mate gedragwetenskappe en 'n verskeidenheid van insette kan suksesvol aangewend word in die verandering van menslike gedrag. sertifikaatprogramme: op grondvlak is daar tans 'n noodoproep om direkte gemeenskapsopvoeding' en -verryking vir die voorkoming van kommunikasiegestremdheid, vir sifting, 7 verwysing en ook basiese verligting van lyding te voorsien. die opleiding van gemeenskapsgesondheidswerkers/gemeenskapsrehabilitasiewerkers kan deur gedesentraliseerde diensleweringsorganisasies soos hospitale sonder noemenswaardige finansiele implikasies behartig word, met die aanbieding van sertifikaatprogramme en indiensopleiding. gemeenskapsleiers en gemeenskapsverpleegkundiges kan hierdie verantwoordelikheid aanvaar sonder dat daar werklik tot hulle werksbelading toegevoeg word. alhoewel hierdie opleiding noodwendig baie beperk sal wees, het ons universiteit 'n verpligting om hierdie soort gemeenskapsopvoeding te steun. dit is ons plig om vir die gemeenskappe waarby ons betrokke is die visstok in die hand te gee, om binne die raamwerk van die "doctrine of informed consent" die gemeenskap te bekragtig om self behoeftes te identifiseer en besluite te neem op grond van oorwoe kennis, wat ons vir hulle behoort te verskaf. in ons sentrum vir aanvullende en alternatiewe kommunikasie word gemeenskapsopvoeding reeds met groot sukses aangewend tot voordeel van die erggestremde. ons word oorval met noodoproepe om hulp, en in die kort bestaan van die sentrum is honderde mense, selfs tot in namibie, se lewenskwaliteit al deur die bemiddeling van persone wat in die gemeenskappe o p g e l e i is, verbeter. d e u r e w o r d o o p g e m a a k vir m o e d e r t a a l o n d e r r i g , t w e e d e t a a l o n d e r r i g en selfs geletterdheid. diplomaprogramme: die volgende vlak van dienslewering behoort egter dieper kennis te dra van kommunikasiegestremdhede, omdat habilitasieen rehabilitasieprosedures 'n groter mate van tegniese kennis en vaardigheid vereis. die technikons kan, in samewerking met die universiteit diplomakursusse daarstel om spraaken gehoorgemeenskapswerkers en spraak-taal-gehoorterapieassistente op te lei om in konsultasie met, of onder die toesig van gekwalifiseerde terapeute tegnieke vir kommunikasieherstel aan te wend. hierdie diens kan ook tyden geldbesparend wees en 'n verhoging in toerekenbare en doeltreffende mensekrag teweegbring. weer eens het ons universiteit die verantwoordelikheid om te sorg dat die opleiding toepaslik is, sodat 'n verantwoordbare diens, wat veilig vir die publiek is, gelewer kan word. daar is ook reeds bewyse dat hierdie samewerking suksesvol kan wees, omdat van ons personeel reeds betrokke is by die opleiding van spraaken gehoorgemeenskapswerkers en gehoorapparaatakoestici, wat 'n baie spesifieke diens aan die gemeenskap bied. universiteitsgrade: maar die universiteit se primere taak bly die opvoeding van spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe persone wat al die komplekse fasette van menslike kommunikasie en kommunikasiepatologie ken en kan hanteer. hierdie persone moet 'n bree basies-wetenskaplike en 'n dieptevakwetenskaplike kennis he om probleme te kan identifiseer en te kan oplos. hulle moet bereid en in staat wees om die uiteindelike verantwoordelikheid vir die lewering van effektiewe diens aan alle kommunikasiegestremdes en hulppersoneel op hulle skouers te neem. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 isabel c. uys indien ons kursus dan, soos ek beweer het, van hoogstaande gehalte was, waarom het ons oor die afgelope paar j a a r so drasties geherkurrikuleer? waarom het ons die magistergraad geherstruktureer? kursusse is opgestel binne die raamwerk van nuutgeformuleerde modelle vir opleiding en dienslewering. dit is proaktiewe optrede om te verseker dat hierdie kursusse voorsiening maak vir dienslewering vir die toekoms. baccalaureusgrade: alhoewel ons nie met die beskikbare fasiliteite en bestaande personeelkorps onmiddellik meer voorgraadse studente kon keur nie, word verskeie van die dilemmas reeds aangespreek: sekere teoretiese kursusse word oopgestel, sodat studehte uit ander verwante vakgebiede kennis kan verwerf oor menslike kommunikasie en kommunikasiepatologie. die doel hiervan is gemeenskapsverryking en -opvoeding, waardeur die kommunikasiegestremde indirek sal baat vind. studente uit al die verskillende taalen kultuurgroepe, wat aan die vereistes vir keuring voldoen, word gewerf om die tekort aan terapeute in sekere sektore aan te vul. 'n vakgerigte akademiese ontwikkelingsprogram word in die departement aangebied om studente, wie se moedertaal nie afrikaans is nie, te ondersteun. daar word van alle studente verwag om op die 200vlak 'n afrikataal aan te bied, sodat hulle beter voorberei kan word om in verskillende taalen kultuurgemeenskappe met ander te kan saamwerk om'n effektiewe diens te lewer. gemeenskapsdiens, wat die basis vorm vir onderrig en navorsing, vind plaas in 'n verskeidenheid van taalen kultuuromgewings. studente word dus reeds op voorgraadse vlak toegerus met vaardighede om gedei'nstitusionaliseerd intervensieprogramme te implementeer. alhoewel die kursus besonder swaar gelaai is, word spraak-taalpatologie en oudiologie steeds gesamentlik as hoofvakke aangebied. hierdie kombinasie word nie by alle universiteite oorsee aangebied nie, en tog pleit beroepsverenigings oorsee al vir die afgelope twee dekades dat spraak-taalterapie en oudiologie as 'n enkel-professie beoefen moet word (feldman, 1981). tesame hiermee, is weggedoen met die afwykinggerigte fragmentasie in h i e r d i e professionele opleiding. die menslike kommunikasieproses word deurgaans as uitgangspunt in die onderrig gesien. spraak-, taalen gehoorafwykings word gesien as aspekte van menslike kommunikasie en intervensie word gerig op kommunikasie-patologie. dit is die rede waarom die lomp benaming van die departement ontoepaslik en verwarrend is en waarom 'n naams·' verandering na die departement kommunikasiepatologie oorsee deur meeste universiteite, en plaaslik deur betrokkenes op verskillende vlakke, ook in die vakvereniging en beroepsraad gesteun word. hierdie opleiding van generaliste hou die voordeel in, dat gegradueerdes toegerus word om alle soorte kommunikasiegestremdhede onder alle verskillende omstandighede effektief te hanteer. met in agneming van die uitdagings en bedreigings in die toekoms, is die beroepsfunksies van die gegradueerde ondersoek. dit het duidelik geword dat samewerking met verwante dissiplines en betrokkenes in die gemeenskap onontbeerlik is. daarom word daar in die opleiding nie slegs aandag gegee aan diagnose en terapie nie, maar ook aan gemeenskapsopvoeding (veral met die oog op voorkoming); beraad; konsultasie; supervisie; navorsing; onderrig; bestuur. graduandi word dus voorberei om ook as besluitnemers, konsultante, bestuurders en werkgewers op te tree. maar die uitdagings van beroepsbeoefening in die toekoms, moes ook op ander vlakke aangespreek word. daar moes voorsiening gemaak word vir spesialisdienste in die geval van baie komplekse en moeilik hanteerbare gestremdhede. magister en doktorale grade: die departement bied reeds die afgelope drie jaar 'n gestruktureerde magisterprogram aan met die oog op die a k a d e m i e s e o n d e r r i g van s p e s i a l i s t e in die wetenskappe van kommunikasiepatologie en navorsingsmetodologie. ten spyte van die feit dat dit nie 'n professionele, kliniese graad is nie, kan die sukses van die magisterprogram reeds gemeet word aan die hoogstaande gehalte studente, ook van ander taalen kultuurgroepe wat inskryf en kwalifiseer (ook graduandi en personeel van ander universiteite); oudstudente wat op professionele vlak in uitvoerende poste bevorder word; wat hulle navorsingsvaardighede inspan om die effektiwiteit van dienslewering te verhoog; wat internasionaal aanvaarbare navorsingsen publikasie-uitsette lewer. 'n volgende toekomsideaal is om gespesialiseerde, professionele magisterprogramme in te stel vir persone met 'n algemene, maar toepaslike baccalaureusgraad. so, byvoorbeeld, kan gegradueerde onderwysers op nagraadse vlak 'n magister verwerf, kwalifiseer as spraak-taalterapeut of oudioloog, beperkte registrasie toegelaat word en ook 'n spraak-taalterapeutiese diens aan die skoolgaande kind lewer. die onderwyser by 'n skool vir gehoorgestremdes sal met hierdie kwalifikasie weer 'n belangrike bydrae tot die gehoorgestremde gemeenskap kan lewer. j hierdie soort opleiding is dan ook algemene praktyk by verskeie amerikaanse universiteite. 1 een van die implikasies van uitbreiding van opleiding is fasiliteite en personeel. deur akkreditering van diensleweringsinstansies, kan die universiteit gebruik maak van daardie personeel om die universiteitspersoneel op 'n tydelik-deeltydse basis by te staan in die kliniese opleiding van studente. die voordeel hieraan verbonde is dat die universiteit al hoe meer gebruik sal kan maak van afstandsonderrig. nagraadse studente sal steeds hulle beroepe kan beoefen terwyl hulle deur middel van afstandsonderrig die teoretiese onderrig ontvang. die praktiese, kliniese opleiding kan by geakkrediteerde praktyke, onder die supervisie van gekwalifiseerde spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe plaasvind in hulle woongebiede.' inspeksie van opleiding deur die beroepsraad en e k s a m i n e r i n g deur die universiteit sal steeds die versekering bied dat die standaard van onderrig bevredig. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) kommunikasiepatologie: onderrig vir die toekoms 9 voortgesette onderrig: waar die westerse model die afgelope drie dekades vir die opleiding van spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe aan die universiteit van pretoria gebruik is, is 'n herorientasie tot die afrikakonteks gebiedend noodsaaklik. ons oudstudente het, met 'n ingeskerpte etiese bewustheid, deur selfstudie en selfverryking hulle basiese opleiding aangevul om te voldoen aan die eise wat die beroep in suid-afrika aan hulle stel. maar nou, meer as ooit tevore, het die universiteit die verantwoordelikheid teenoor hierdie oudstudente om die nuwe raamwerke bekend te stel. voortgesette onderrig in die vorm van konferensies, werkswinkels en simposiums is, soos in die verlede noodsaaklik, maar oudstudente behoort ook aangemoedig te word om vir nie-graaddoeleindes modules van die nuwe voorgraadse kursus by te woon. die personeel en studente het die verdere verantwoordelikheid om opleiding en dienslewering gedurig te weeg deur navorsing, en hierdie bevindings bekend te stel ter bevordering van die wetenskap en die beroep en die verryking van die gemeenskap. ook op hierdie gebied pleit ek vir hoe standaarde, maar bevraagteken die kriteria wat gestel word vir e r k e n n i n g van publikasies oor navorsingsbevindings. navorsingsbevindings wat geplaas word in ongesubsidieerde publikasies, kan gemeet word aan ander, ook streng wetenskaplike en kommunikasiekundige kriteria en juis as gevolg daarvan van groter waarde wees. geen wonder dat davies (1993, p. 14) die volgende se nie: "too often scientists publish only in highly technical journals and the message does not reach the people that count." die personeel in die departement is voorberei daarop om hierdie verantwoordelikhede te aanvaar, omdat hulle met behoud van die eiesoortige aard en met uitnemendheid kommunikasiepatologie as wetenskap deur onderrig, navorsing en gemeenskapsdiens wil beoefen en 'n studentekorps wil ontwikkel en vorm deur die oordrag van dissiplinegerigte kennis en k u n d i g h e i d ten einde 'n verantwoordelike en betekenisvolle beroepsgerigte bydrae tot die samelewing te lewer. is dit die moeite werd? ek laat dit in u midde met die woorde van beukelman en garrett (1988, p. 104): "speechlessness is not a loss of life, but a loss of access to life." verwysings aron, m.l. (1984). introduction. proceedings of conference on community work in speech and hearing therapy, 1-5. beukelman, d.r. & garrett, k.l. (1988). augmentative and alternative communication for adults with acquired severe communication disorders. augmentative and alternative communication, 4, 104-121. cole, p.r. (1986). i want to shape my own future. how about you? asha, 28(9), 41-42. crossley, s. (1986). how to train clinicians to work with culturally different clients. communiphoh, 280, 2-11. cunnington, b. (1985). the process of educating and developing managers for the year 2000. journal of management development, 4, 66-79. davies,r. (1993). on their way to the top. scientech, 14. delaney, c. & malan, k. (1984). community speech and hearing therapy:some questions before answers. proceedings of conference on community work in speech and hearing therapy, 73-84. (1987). disability in the republic of south africa. main report. 1. pretoria: department of national health and population development. feldman, a.s. (1981). the challenge of autonomy. asha, 941945. flower, r.m. (1984). delivery of speech-language pathology and audiology services. baltimore: williams & wilkins. johnson, s. & wilson, l. (1987). the one minute sales person. glasgow: william collins. rieber, r.w. & brubaker, r.s. (1966). speech pathology. amsterdam: north-holland publ. co. uys, i.c. & hugo, s. r. (1989). 'n veelvlakevaluasie as grondslag vir die herkurrikulering van 'n professionele universiteitskursus. evaluering in tersiere onderwys. stellenbosch: buvo & savnoho. 259-274. uys, i.c. & hugo, s.r. (1990). kurrikulering vir spraakheelkunde en oudiologie. pretoria: universiteit van pretoria. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 boeke is wen met kennis! van schaik boekhandel is jou voorste boekhandelaar met 'n reuse verskeidenheid akademiese boeke, lekker-leesboeke, rekenaargidse, skryfware en geskenke! besoek ons vandag en deel in die magdom kennis. virdaardie spesiale geleentheid of kopkrap is ons daar! want ons en boeke is een! one lever dienskeer op keer! van schaik boekhandel nedbank forum h/v burnetten festivalstraat hatfield 0083 β (012) 43-3717 f (012) 43-6536 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a markedness analysis of initial consonant clusters in aphasic phonological impairment: a case study. lesley wolk, b.a. (sp. & η. th.) (witwatersrand) dept. speech therapy and audiology, baragwanath hospital, johannesburg. summary the purpose of this study was to assess both the theoretical and clinical value of markedness theory in phonological impairment in aphasia. a markedness analysis was carried out on initial consonant clusters in a single aphasic adult, at two points during the spontaneous recovery phase. results revealed systematic, rule-governed behaviour, reflecting similar linguistic trends, in terms of natural segments and natural processes, on both testing occasions. some inadequacies of the distinctive feature approach are discussed. the findings of this study suggest that a markedness analysis may be extremely useful for the analysis and treatment of phonological disorders in aphasia. opsomming die doel van hierdie studie was om beide die teoretiese en kliniese waarde van die merkbaarheids teorie ('markedness theory') in fonologiese gebrek in afasie te evalueer. 'n merkbaarheids analise is uitgevoer op inisiele konsonant groepe in 'n enkele volwassene met afasie op twee tydstippe tydens die spontane herstel periode. resultate het sistematiese reel-gebonde gedrag tydens albei toetsgeleenthede getoon, wat 'n soortgelyke taalpatroon weerspieel het in terme van beide natuurlike segmente en prosesse. sommige tekortkominge van die benadering van die onderskeidings kenmerke word bespreek. die bevindings van hierdie studie toon aan dat 'n merkbaarheids analise baie nuttig mag wees in die analise en behandeling van fonologiese abnormaliteite in afasie. ' central to the analysis and treatment of aphasic phonological impairment is the application of linguistics. luria assumes the phoneme to be the basic unit of all spoken language.21 this statement encompasses two fundamental justifications for the study of phonology as discussed by spreen,30 firstly for its own intrinsic value, and secondly as the presence of articulatory substitutions may interrupt the investigation and treatment of higher order linguistic events. there are two major characteristics of aphasic phonological impairment; the substitution process in which the selection and substitution are two faces of the same operation,'6 and the disruption of phonemic sequencing. studies of articulatory difficulties in aphasia have dealt mainly with substitution phenomena, and have placed little emphasis on the combination process. in view of the fact that patients with articulation problems show particular difficulty in the production of consonant clusters, 1 ' 2 0 ' 2 9 an in-depth investigation of this was felt to be an interesting medium through which these dimensions (selection and sequencing) could be explored. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol.25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 lesley wolk 'apraxia of speech' is a term commonly used to describe articulation difficulties in aphasia. it is clearly differentiated in this study from oral apraxia, which is defined by derenzi and vignolo8 as: the inability to perform voluntary movements with the muscles of the larynx, pharynx, tongue, lips and cheeks; although automatic movements of the same muscles are preserved. a basic controversy seems to emerge from the literature, reflecting two opposing views about articulation disorders in 'apraxia of speech.' several s t u d i e s 5 ' 7 ' 2 9 reveal that articulation errors in 'apraxia of speech' are primarily inconsistent, reflecting a lack of systematic relationships between error and target sounds; while other w r i t e r s 1 ' 9 ' 1 3 ' 1 6 ' 3 3 have emphasized the systematic nature of articulation errors. specific attention has been drawn to the possibility of applying distinctive — feature theory to phonemic errors in an aphasic adult.19 it seems that the distinctive — feature (d.f.) approach may have certain inadequacies. while most researches in speech pathology have found a feature framework valuable as both a descriptive and therapeutic tool, 2 5 ' 3 1 it has come under attack by walsh,34 hyman1 4 and others, who feel that a d.f. approach has as its principle object an abstract, idealized level of language. it is primarily concerned with the system and structures of phonological oppositions rather than with concrete manifestations of human speech. recently, another linguistic theory has received attention in the exploration of children's phonological systems. this theory is called 'markedness theory' (m.t.) based on the underlying theoretical assumptions of natural phonology proposed by david stampe. 6 ' 1 4 while it has been claimed that the notion of markedness is highly relevant to the analysis of aphasic speech, in agreement with kagan,1 9 the writer felt that it had not been placed in perspective linguistically. the 'naturalness' of certain segments in phonological systems can be captured through the notion of 'markedness.' two phonemes or phoneme sequences (segments) are differentiated by considering one of them unmarked (u) for a particular feature and the other marked (m) for that feature. the basic assumption is that the (u) member represents the more natural state, whereas the sum of the (m) features represents the complexity of that phoneme. 2 ' 4 relative complexity is derived from perceptual distinctiveness, frequency of occurrence of certain phonemes among languages of the world, and physiological considerations such as deviations of the vocal apparatus from a phonetically neutral position.2'2 4 within the framework of natural ι phonology, it is assumed that the process of acquisition of the phonetic pattern of language, is therefore a process of inhibiting natural processes.6 i the underlying rationale for this study was to investigate whether an application of m.t. would firstly bje of theoretical value in yielding a better understanding into speech processes resulting from neurological disruption, and secondly to assess its clinical use. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia 83 method s u b j e c t (s) the s used in this study was a white, south african, english-speaking adult male, aged 56 years. severe expressive aphasia resulted from an intracerebral haemorrhage in the distribution of the middle cerebral artery, which was further confirmed by an emi scan. the s selected fulfilled the following criteria: 1) he was diagnosed as aphasic by a neurologist and a speech therapist, and was assessed as having 'apraxia of speech' (as defined by deal and darley7). this was further confirmed by a formal rating on the boston diagnostic test of aphasia.'2 2) the s demonstrated articulation difficulties, particularly on consonant clusters. 3) dysarthria was ruled out as being etiologically related to the articulation difficulties. 4) oro-facial apraxia was ruled out as being causally related to the articulatory errors. 5) peripheral hearing and vision were within normal limits. 6) the s's mother tongue was english. 7) the s had an adequate premorbid acquisition of speech and language. a i m s 1) to determine whether articulation errors on initial-stem consonant clusters in a single case of aphasia, can be characterized by systematic, rule-governed behaviour. 2) to assess the applicability of markedness theory to phonological impairment in an aphasic in terms of the following:a) are unmarked (u) phonemes and phoneme sequences substituted for marked (m) ones more than the reverse? b) do successive attempts at self-correction reveal a progression towards the target segment in terms of complexity values (u · m), or are they random? c) are predictable patterns set up in one testing confirmed by another? d) can articulation errors be characterized by natural phonological processes? 3) to discuss the appropriateness of the term 'apraxia of speech' used to describe phonological impairment in aphasia, with reference to the general findings and two further issues:i) whether 'apraxia of speech' is purely a motor disorder, or whether there is a sensory component. ii) whether 'apraxia of speech' is purely an articulatory disorder or whether there is an inter-action with other levels of language function. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 84 markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia t e s t s a n d p r o c e d u r e pre-tests 1) the boston diagnostic aphasia examination.12 this test was used in order to obtain a profile of scores on various linguistic dimensions. administration followed the procedures laid down in the test manual. 2) a screening pure-tone audiometric test. this test was administered at a reference threshold level of 15 db (iso 1964). the s responded adequately at all frequencies, indicating that hearing was within normal limits. 3) a neurological examination. a detailed battery of neurological tests were carried out by a neurologist, which served to satisfy those criteria not assessed on the boston examination and to further confirm others. 4) auditory discrimination tests. a) the goldman-fristoe test of auditory discrimination." this test was administered in order to assess the s's competence for discriminating between single consonants. administration procedures followed those laid down in the test manual. b) in order to evaluate the s's ability to recognize cluster versus noncluster segments, an informal test of 50 minimal pair words was devised (eg. tea/tree). 5) auditory memory span. an informal test of auditory memory span was administered using (a) a series of digits, (b) a series of related words and (c) a series of unrelated words. this was felt to be a crucial aspect in the consideration of consonant cluster production. 6) the goldman-fristoe test of articulation.10 part of this test was administered in order to assess basic trends occurring on single consonants within the context of meaningful words. this was felt to provide a basis for the analysis of consonant clusters. tests two approaches were used for elicitation of speech for linguistic analysis: a) controlled elicitation through naming (naming t a s k . y b) uncontrolled elicitation through the collection of spontaneous speech (spontaneous tasks.) a) naming task while many articulation tests are available, none have been designed for in-depth phonological analysis of consonant clusters. a task was thus devised, comprising 106 stimulus cards representing words with initial-stem consonant cluster sequences. stimulus cards consisted of large coloured pictures mounted on white cards. the words the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia 85 to be elicited were written on the back of each card for the purpose of the tester. criteria for selection of words: 28 initial-stem consonant clusters in english are listed.18 each one was elicited in more than one stimulus word, ranging between two and six different phonemic environments. the environment was varied with respect to both vowels and consonants in the immediate phonemic vicinity. an attempt was made to take into account word length and syllable structure in devising the test items. experimental manipulation of words: stimulus pictures were restricted by the following constraints i) phonological rules governing the combination and sequencing of phonemes in english, and ii) those words which could be pictorially represented, for the reason that the use of imitation was deliberately avoided so as to prevent contamination of results by providing a model. in addition, an informal test of 20 words comprising final-stem consonant clusters was devised. they were divided into two groups of 10 each. in the first group, the consonant clusters were purely phonological constructions (pc) such as / s p / in "lisp". in the second group, the final consonant clusters were morphological combinations such as /st/ in "crossed" or /ts/ in "pots". cvcc words were restricted to combinations of / s / + voiceless plosive (sp, st, sk) or (ps, ts, ks), as these are the only two elements comprising a final-stem cluster which occur in both (pc) and (mc). each word was tested in two to three different phonemic environments. this test was included so as to assess superficially whether any other subtle linguistic deficits were operating apart from a phonological impairment. rationale for use of meaningful words: meaningful words were used as opposed to nonsense syllables, for the reasons that firstly in all speaking situations, sound and meaning are inseparable and phonological errors are thus necessarily interactive with both semantic and syntactic levels of speech. secondly, by observing errors in meaningful words, interesting phenomena such as assimilations and reversals within words and over word boundaries become obvious, and act as potential cues.for both diagnosis and treatment. furthermore, the idea which permeates throughout this study, is that of naturalness, and nonsense syllables are in themselves an artificial form of speech. b) spontaneous tasks i) thirty minutes of an open-ended conversation was elicited, concerning the patient's illness, work and general issues. ii) three thematic apperception test (tat) cards were used (numbers 1, 2, 13b) in order to elicit a spontaneous story. iii) six large coloured picture cards were carefully chosen in order to elicit several words with initial consonant clusters, within the context of connected speech. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 86 lesley wolk in addition, much of the spontaneous samples resulted from the s's misnaming of test items and interjected spontaneous speech. it was felt, that many previous studies in 'apraxia of speech' could be criticized for their use of nonsense syllable repetition tasks and specific naming tasks, thus failing to stimulate communicative language. a d m i n i s t r a t i o n o f t e s t p r o c e d u r e s : two testing sessions took place with a six week interval between them. testing a was carried out six weeks post-stroke, and testing β was carried out three months post-stroke, which was considered to have taken place at a point of neurological stability.28 the second testing served (a) to assess improvement of articulatory performance during the period of spontaneous recovery; (b) to confirm phonological patterns set up in the first testing; and (c) to control for chance factors. for the naming tasks, the s was instructed to name the pictures on presentation of each card. they were presented in a random order, but attempts were made not to present two words with the same consonant cluster successively. stimulus words with final-stem consonant clusters were elicited in a similar manner. for the spontaneous tasks, the s was instructed to tell a story with a beginning, a middle and an end in response to each individual tat test card, and to describe each of the six test picture cards discussed above. testings took place at different times of the day, so as to obtain a representative sample of performance. they were carried out for short periods of time (30 — 40 minutes per session) which enabled the tester to control as far as possible for variables such as fatigue, anxiety, motivation, mood and other external factors. in testing b, the following pre-tests were re-administered: the goldman-fristoe test of auditory discrimination" and production,10 and the boston diagnostic aphasia examination.12 analysis procedure and scoring: all responses were recorded on a revox tape recorder (model 1132 dolby version). responses were transcribed in broad phonetic transcriptions at the time of testing where possible, and later by several transcribers, thus providing maximum objectivity and accuracy of recorded data. the analysis procedure was carried out along the following dimensions:1. a) a frequency count of correct versus incorrect responses. one point was assigned for the correct production of the cluster on initial attempt. half a point was assigned for the correct production on one of the subsequent attempts, and 0 for the incorrect production ie. where the target was never reached. all points were totalled for each cluster in proportion to the total number (n) of test words for that cluster (which varied according to the number of self-corrections). a percentage was calculated for the total number of correct responses out of the total number of stimulus test words. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia 87 (note: both testing a and β comprised 106 test items each on naming tasks; whereas the criterion for spontaneous tasks, was to extract from the total spontaneous speech sample, between 100106 initial-stem consonant cluster words in order to confirm the findings on the more structured tasks). b) a distribution of errors according to omissions (o), substitutions (s), additions (a) and distortions (d). in c1c2vc syllable structures, it is obvious that one or more of the 4 error categories may occur concurrently in one word. c) a subphonemic feature analysis of substitution errors according to the features of place (p), manner (m), voice (v). overlapping categories were developed so as to accommodate substitutions which were defective in two or more features. (p-m, p-v, m-v, pm-v). this method is suggested by la pointe and johns.20 2. a markedness analysis a) articulation errors on initial consonant clusters were subjected to a markedness analysis by applying marked (m) and unmarked (u) values to every consonant and total complexity values to every consonant cluster sequence. b) the m/u table, devised by the writer, was based on those values suggested by cairns,2 chomsky and halle4 with various modifications. (see table i). c) a 9-distinctive feature (d.f.) matrix was used with values available for the following features: consonantal, sonorant, anterior, coronal, continuant, strident, voice, lateral and nasal. a complexity score was computed by adding the (rti) features in each consonant. feature ρ b f v θ δ t d j 3 t j d 3 m η q 1 r w y h k g s ζ x consonantal u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u u sonorant m m m m m m m m m m m m u u u u m u u u m m m m m anterior u u u u u u u u m m m m u u m u u u m m m m u u m coronal u u u u u . u u u u u u u u u u u u u continuant u u m m m m u u m m u u m m m m m m m m u u m m m strident u u u u u u u u m m m m u u u u u u u u u u m m u voiced u m u m u m u m u m u m u u u u u u u u u m u m u lateral + nasal u u u u u u u u u u u u m m m u u u u u u u u u u 6u 5u 5u 4u 6u 5u 7u 6u 4u 3u 5u 4u5u 6u 5u 7u 6u 6u 6u 5u 6u 5u 5u 4u 5u complexity 1 2 2 3 2 3 1 2 4 5 3 4 2 2 3 1 2 1 2 2 2 3 3 4 3 note: although / x / is not an english phoneme, it is included in the table as the s used this sound in his speech. table i: m/u values for all consonants in english, using a 9 distinctivefeature matrix. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 88 lesley wolk d) the analysis of individual feature change can, therefore, assume one of the following possible patterns m u, m · m, u -> u, u * m. e) the symbols (m), (u) and (mm) were used to refer to the direction of change in terms of complexity values: (m) — the target cluster, or a substitution of equal complexity value to that of the target, (u) — a substituted phoneme or phoneme sequence which has a lower complexity value than that of the target, (mm) — a substituted phoneme or phoneme sequence which has a greater complexity value than that of the target, which is always (m); it reflects a change from less complex to more complex. (note: the reason for assigning (m) values to all target segments is that the ccvc words are already complex in terms of syllable structure.) f) phonological processes of simplification include a more detailed analysis of errors in the following categories: i syllable structure, ii environment, iii substitutions. (see table vi). scoring: model for assigning complexity values. 1) paradigmatic values refer to a discrete value for each phoneme, depending on the sum total of (m) features for each consonant. in this model, v = 0 , c=range between 1 and 5, where p = l , 3 = 5; thus reflecting the 'naturalness' of phonemes relative to articulatory and perceptual complexity. 2) syntagmatic values refer to complexity values assigned according to the syllable structure of a word (or phonemic sequencing) such that cv = (+0) c1c2v = ( + 1) c1c2c3v = (+2) these values are justified in terms of the 'naturalness' of syllable structure. syntagmatic values were applied to all consonant sequences, with the following implications — one of the most common neutralization rules governing consonant sequences in english, is that involving the / s/ phoneme. a feature is said to be 'neutralized' in an environment if the value of the feature in this environment is determined by a sequential restraint,32 a neutralization rule (n-rule) thus specifies that a particular feature or set of features may not have a (m) value in a given environment; ie the complexity value is reduced. in this model, a value of ( 2 ) is thus subtracted from the total complexity value, if / s / occurs in the following environments: i a) in a c1c2 cluster, where c2'varies with a voiceless plosive, b) in c1c2c3 cluster, where / s / is the only phoneme which may occur in position ci, according to the rules of phonemic sequencing in english. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia (the value ~2 was fairly arbitrarily chosen. often an n-rule would require a subtraction of the entire value; however, in this case / s / has a value of 3 so that it would result in a value of 0 which is equal to a vowel in this model. for this reason, only a value of 2 was subtracted). 3) complexity of cluster segments refer to values assigned according to the acquisition of consonant clusters. a definite sequence of acquisition for each cluster has not been given in the literature, as has been done for singletons. however values were based on the broad order of acquisition described by ingram.15 these were divided into 3 groups:i) those acquired by age 4 years (+0) ii) those acquired between ages 4 — 6 years (+1) iii) those acquired by age 7 years (+2) justification: one of the criteria used for assigning (m) values to single consonants, is according to acquisition o r d e r . 2 ' 1 4 it was thus felt necessary to .extend this to consonant clusters. all utterances were thus analyzed by applying all three rules. no. 1 describes complexity of single consonants or singletons; no. 2 refers to the complexity of syllable structure; and no. 3 describes complexity of consonant clusters. note: consonant sequences produced by the s, which were 'nonenglish' sequences, followed rules (1) and (2) but not (3). the identical analysis procedures were applied to the data obtained in testing a and testing b. cluster r-b lends 1-blends s-b lends 2-element blends 3-element blends cluster pr br tr dr kr gr fr θ γ j r pi bl kl gl fl sm sn sp st sk si sw tw kw spl spr str skr skw complexity value 4 5 4 5 5 7 6 7 9 3 4 4 5 5 4 4 3 3 4 7 7 3 4 7 8 7 9 7 t a b l e 11: a list of complexity values for each initial-stem consonant cluster in english. results and discussion (i) the boston diagnostic aphasia examination:the s obtained overall scores of (2) and (3) on testing a and testing β respectively, according to goodglass and kaplan's categories.12 the profile of scores thus served to reflect the interactions of various deficits at a given time, and the improvement from one point in recovery to another. (ii) the s made no errors on traditional tests of auditory discrimination. 1. a) frequency count: analysis of correct versus incorrect responses, revealed an improvement on consonant cluster production during the period of spontaneous recovery. for example, a score of 51,42% was the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 90 lesley wolk obtained on the naming task in testing a, and 65,56 % on the naming task on testing b. 1. b) analysis of omissions (o), substitutions, (s), additions (a) and distortions (d): results clearly illustrated the predominance of (s) errors, followed.^ by (o). (a) and (d) comprised the smaller part of the contribution'* to the total number of errors. the frequency of errors followed the sequence s > ο > d > a which was consistent on all tasks. the fact that patients with 'apraxia of speech' make more errors in substitutions is well supported in the literature.1'3 3 1.c) subphonemic feature analysis: on all tasks, results revealed that errors of place (p) comprised the largest percentage (%), followed by errors of manner (m) and a combination of these (p-m). errors of voice (v) either alone or in combination with others comprised the smaller parts. these results were consistent with the literature. 2 0 ' 3 3 it is interesting that the frequency of errors in children is also seen as ρ > μ > v. 2. a) markedness analysis: a separate 9-d.f. matrix was drawn up for each target consonant cluster and its various substitutions; for the naming and spontaneous tasks on both testing a and testing b. marked (m) and unmarked (u) values were assigned to each consonant in the matrix (transposed from table i). the total number of m's were added so as to reveal the complexity value of each consonant on the paradigmatic axis. by applying the 'rules' discussed in analysis procedure, total complexity values were assigned to each cluster sequence, thus incorporating both paradigmatic and syntagmatic levels. a markedness analysis of all errors revealed that many of the utterances consisted of phonemes and phoneme sequences in which the value of the features in the target segments were retained (m). when a change occurred, most of the target segments were replaced by (u) segments and only a few were replaced by (mm) segments. for example, in naming a, out of a total of 285 utterances, 128 (44,92%) were replaced by (u) segments, and only 49 (17,19%) were replaced by (mm) segments. the remainder revealed no change (m). clearly the data did not support a statement that the s's articulation errors always reflect a change from more to less complex. however, they did suggest that when a change occurs, it is more likely that the direction will be from more to less complex (m -j> u). a number of suggestions were made to explain those occasions where (mm) segments replaced (m) segments, such as assimilations, slips of the tongue, or complexity over the entire word. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia 91 sm (3—2)+2 + l = (4) total complexity feature s m s ρ m cons u u u u u son m u m m u ant u u u u u cor u u cont m m m u m stri m u m u u voice u u u u u lat nas u m u u m complexity 3 2 3 1 2 word cluster total c m/u 1 smell m 2 u 4 u—>m sm 4 m 2 smarm 2 u ties u—>m ties sp 3 u u—>m 3 smiling sp 3 u sp 3 u 4 smoke sm 4 m total c m/u : total complexity : marked or unmarked segments. table iii: example of a 9 d.f. matrix computed for the initial consonant cluster /sm/, and its various substitutions. from table iii it can be seen that there is a tendency for (u) segments to replace (m) ones, which is consistent with blumstein' and kagan's19 findings. it should be noted that kagan dealt primarily with singletons, blumstein with consonant clusters on syllable structure only, and neither provided a detailed account of relative complexity values. it thus makes it difficult to incorporate their findings in the discussion of the present data. cairns3 found that the child's substitutions may be characterized by a shift towards the (u) value for a particular feature. she felt that the m/u values of features are of more value in accounting for substitutions, than only the +/— dimension. 2. b) analysis of self-correction: results indicate that there was a stronger tendency for a progression to occur, rather than a nonprogression. ie. as the s approximated the target segment, he substituted phonemes or phoneme sequences of increasing complexity. not only do results on spontaneous tasks confirm those on naming tasks, but also results on testing β revealed that the tendency for a pr.ogression in terms of complexity values to occur, becomes more firmly established towards the end of spontaneous recovery. the example 'scratch' in table iv clearly demonstrates the process of self-correction for the cluster /skr/. the initial attempt is a single consonant / l / with a low complexity value equal to 1, reflecting the most natural syllable structure cvc. the second and third attempts are consonant clusters / k l / and /si/ respectively with complexity values of 4 and 7 respectively. on the fourth attempt, the target segment is reached, namely the most complex syllable structure c1c2c3vc where /skr/ has a total complexity value equal to 9. johns and darley17 characterize the 'variability' of phonemic production in 'apraxia of speech,' as unrelated, additive, substitutions, repetitions and blocks, groping through repeated efforts towards the correct production. this finding contradicts that of the present study, which provides evidence for a pattern of successive attempts at self-correction die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 92 lesley wolk skr ( 3 2 ) + ( 2 + 2 ) + 2 + 2 = (9) feature s k r 1 k 1 s 1 cons u u u u u u u u son m m m u m u m u ant u m u u m u u u cor u u u u u u u u cont m u m m u m m m stri m u u u u u m u voice u u u u u u u u lat + + + nas u u u u u u u u compl. 3 2 2 1 2 1 3 1 total complexity wordcluster total m/u scratch 1 1 u" kl 4 u si u—> m si 7 u u—> m skr 9 m table iv: example of a 9 d.f. matrix computed for the initial-stem consonant cluster /skr/, to show the process of self-correction. with a definite link or 'relatedness' between error production and desired production. these results were felt to be extremely significant for two reasons: firstly it provided an opportunity to observe dynamic linguistic processing, and secondly it provided further information about perception. the fact that the s was able to monitor his own productions showed that to some extent the perceptual mechanism is functioning. however, that this process of selfcorrection was somewhat less than perfect, resulting in further substitution errors seemed to point to a subtle deficit in the sensori-motor feedback loop. the interesting phenomenon which seems to arise is that there is a discrepancy between performance on a purely perceptual task and performance on a task involving an interaction between perception and production. the implications of this were felt to go beyond the scope of this study. however, it may be a worthwhile point for future investigation, which might provide further insight into theories of speech perception and production. pr (1 + 2) + 1 (4) total complexity word testing a testing β cluster total c m/u cluster total c m/u 1 prize ρ 1 u pi 3 u f 2 u pr 4 / m pl 3 u / / pr 4 m 2 pram pi . 3 u pi 3 u pr 4 m 3 present pr 4 m pr 4 m table v: an example of cluster / p r / from naming task a and naming task b, to show the predictable course of recovery. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia 93 table v provides an illustration of the predictable patterns set up in testing a which are confirmed in testing b. the same progression occurred on both testings ie. u-> m, and fewer attempts were required in testing b, before reaching the target segments. for example, in the word 'prize', there were four attempts towards the target segment in testing a ranging from / ρ / — (1) to / p r / — (4). assuming the pattern to be a range from 1 — 4 in terms of complexity, one could predict a substitution of a phoneme or phoneme sequence comprising these phonemes, of values 2 — 4, as approximations before reaching the target segment. in testing β the first approximation was / p i / with a complexity value of 3, after which •the target cluster / p r / was reached. this finding seems to have relevance for assessment and as a therapeutic aid. the ability to predict the outcome of each phonetic process is pointed out as another important difference between jakobson and stampe's theories. jakobson's16 account of d.f. theory has a universal hierarchy of features arranged in a strict pattern of successive branchings, which are viewed as relative properties of a phoneme, defined within a network of oppositions. by contrast, stampe 6 ' 1 4 sees a dynamic system consisting of these innate rules of natural processes, rather than a static hierarchy. from an informal analysis, similar trends were noted on singletons, in initial, medial and final positions, which provided further confirmation for results above. an observation of the data showed that similar error trends tended to occur on different clusters comprising the same elements. for example, in the clusters sp, pr, spr, the following substitutions were noted to occur in testings a and b: b/sp, br/spr, f/sp, f/pr, pl/pr, spl/spr. these results thus served to validate and confirm the findings on initial-stem consonant clusters and further suggested that some underlying system of internalized rules might be governing articulation errors. ' some general points: i) the s was seen to correctly produce more of those clusters acquired earlier in normal child development than those of later acquisition. results also corresponded with children's progress from u ^ m in their learning process. ii) it is interesting to note than when the s was struggling to produce a particular cluster, he frequently used several self-devices to aid the production. for example, to spell the word out aloud which was seen to facilitate correct production immediately, or to spontaneously incorporate the stimulus word in a sentence ie. an attempt to produce it in natural running speech. iii) several error productions could be interpreted as having semantic confusions, yet are fully explicable on a phonological level, eg. 'kribs' for twins, 'god' for dog, 'grub' for mug and 'breast' for dressed. 2. c) analysis of errors according to natural phonological processes the major processes were subdivided into fourteen categories, thus extending those used by blumstein' and hatfield.13 table vi die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lesley wolk represents a summary of all natural phonological processes, with a few examples from the raw data, to illustrate the type of errors produced on cvc and ccvc words. the findings indicate that certain processes seemed to occur more frequently than others in naming and spontaneous tasks on both testings a and b. i) syllable structure: cluster reduction occurred most frequently, representing a form of simplification = ccvc-> cvc. ii) environment: there was a general tendency for regressive assimilations (r.a.) to occur more frequently than progressive assimilations (p.a.) on all tasks. metatheses were less common, but when they did occur, reversals of syllables occurred more often than reversals of phonemes. this finding may be interpreted in terms of m.t, in that such rules of consonant harmony increase the redundancy of the articulatory instruction of the word so that 'gog' would be easier than 'dog'·.6'32 iii) substitutions: the processes of fronting (f) and stopping (s) were clearly the most frequent where f > s. many utterances were seen to include an overlap of stopping (plosivization) and fronting within the same segment. it would appear that while (f) is more common, there is a marked tendency to substitute velar sounds, particularly the voiceless plosive / k / . this is consistent with hatfield13 who reports some interchangeability between / t / and / k / in both singletons and clusters. generally, the above findings are well supported in the literature on aphasic phonological impairment.1'1 3 from a brief comparison with phonological acquisition in the child, similar linguistic trends were seen to occur. apart from the consistent findings of cluster reduction, 1 5 ' 2 6 other processes of syllable simplification have been found to occur in the child ie. syllable closure (cv-> cvc), and the insertion of an epenthetic schwa thus splitting the cluster (clc2-> cvc).15 environmental errors, such as metatheses and assimilations, where regressive assimilation was greater than progressive assimilation were found to occur in the child. 1 5 ' 2 6 the various substitution processes found to operate in the s, are also reported to occur in the child, such as: fronting^ stopping, denasalization, gliding, frication, nasal and velar substitutions.15 overall results of 2a, b, c, d indicate that the general trends occurred on both natural segments and natural processes. stampe discusses the repeated use of strategies or natural phonological processes in the child. its importance would seem to be the fact that it is the fiijst theory to account for both the learning of the phonetic repertoire and of phonological relations.14 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia 95 ir> νΐ s b (n 1, 06 po s. sb m co coo'· (n po s. sb cn i 1 ,0 6 tfi w x i§ ο α. σ σ n b cn 1, 85 i ο ' ζ po s. n b cn m <> ζ ο po s. n b it) | 4, 63 tfi w x i§ •ό of) .ξ 5 s a co 1, 59 i η ζ ο •ό c μ κ « sa 18 ,5 1 < υ 5 u^ •ό π υ ω sa co i 1, 39 w οο ο (λ cfl n a ο ο ο" os uσ* μ £ ν α m 21 ,7 4 < υ 5 u^ ϊ = n a r| 4, 35 in e rr or | e rr or br /g r t/ k e rr or f/ sw f/ kw /m ai n / /s ad a 1 ' sb \ λ ir ε sb τ (ν /m ai n / /s ad a 1 ' sb in in \ο po s. sb .rco d c; e g / m ai n / /s ad a 1 ' sb po s. —• n b ^ ο e g ζ ο jj £ nb (n oo ο ζ n b t co' υ fi 1 ,5 9 33 n b ^ α 3 ο < in co υ fi < co 1, 59 ω sa cn oo α 3 ο •ό ε re -ο α a < in go •«t co η £ n a pj χ η < sa u 1 α 3 ο ε re -ο α a oo < j vc η £ n a ο ο pj χ η < c t i2 2 w < ζ cn t > vc pj χ η < < ζ in ^ η ω s ω μ 03 uo ο » | ο, ο e rr or pw /p r w /r υ υ ζ ο ο ζ 1, 60 ε ω c ο ω ζ ο sb ιη ρ s b co 1, 60 ε ω c ο υ v) sa ^ co \λ s b — υ ο α . n b i n α . υ v) sa t -cr. cc n b fn < θ! ο α . n b ( ν go (λ n a ο ο ο t -cr. cc n b i. n a ο ο cr. sa go (n ο ελ τ os e sa go o* c; ελ (ν < ο sa o* os ω η ί tr ou ta x < ζ ιτ) co • < s b 7, 45 pj > re ο 2 £ < ζ go 11 ,1 8 d j υ ο • crt £3 ^ χ ν > £ ο α . χ ) < \ α . υ 3 n b 2, 77 δ ω ίλ [λ υ £ ο α . χ ) < \ α . υ 3 s a r") (n 12 ,1 6 ω ο η < _ι | 00 υ οέ s 3 sa r") (n 12 ,1 6 ο ζ s b i 0, 53 ο η < _ι | 00 υ οέ n a 1, 59 < ρ < n b r") 2, 77 < ελ w or d ε ο χ. 2 sa r3, 71 ω η ελ d ϊ so ng s ν α i 0, 63 > '(λ λ ιυ ο -σ sa 2, 65 < ζ ω α w or d ε € d re π χ) n a t 2, 48 j υ ο ν ο 00 00 ο £ n a ττ 2, 48 < ζ ω α δ π i ω ο \ ω ζ ο m co 20 ,7 5 po s. ξ ι sb co [1 6, 49 ζ ο po s. sb go 4, 25 ι υ d q ω os • os ω η d j υ ο α. / n b i ιπ (ν 23 ,1 5 ο • ζ po s. ξ ι n b i r.6 ,4 9 η d η po s. n b tt 12 ,9 7 ι υ d q ω os • os ω η d j υ w or d gr in ni ng br ok en sa r(ν 14 ,2 9 α. α. ο w or d sa y de ci de < ελ τ co 17 ,1 8 ελ m d w or d <λ ο τ ι < in tt 2, 12 ι υ d q ω os • os ω η d j υ w or d gr in ni ng br ok en ν α i co co 20 ,5 π ύο w or d sa y de ci de < ζ fo m). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia 97 3) articulation errors could be characterized by natural phonological processes. 4) trends observed in testing a were confirmed in testing b, and those occurring on naming tasks were confirmed on spontaneous tasks, ie. predictable patterns set up in one testing were confirmed by another. slight variability, which was seen to occur within strict linguistic classes, was noted. evidence thus seems to point to an organized 'system', less variable than a random set of articulation disorders. the appropriateness of the term 'apraxia of speech' was questioned in view of the following three conclusions: a) that errors could be characterized linguistically. b) that rather than a purely motor disorder, there might be a mild orosensory perceptual component c) the possibility of subtle levels of language breakdown on a morphological level, operating together with the phonological impairment. it would thus appear to be a condition inseparable from the general aphasic disorder, and should be both assessed and treated in the light of the total syndrome. overall results seem to demonstrate the applicability of natural phonology (np) to phonological impairment in aphasia. the apparent bias of n.p. is clearly that of 'production'. the perceptual data in the present study are not adequately dealt with in the conceptual framework of n.p., which places certain limitations on its general use. however, it is felt that in conjunction with other approaches, m.t. may be highly applicable to aphasia, particularly for analysis and treatment of production errors. theoretically, an attempt has been made to extrapolate from m.t for the aphasic s as it was originally developed to explain acquisition of phonology in the child. from the conclusions, possibly one may view the phonological breakdown in the following way: where the child learns to inhibit natural segments and processes in the acquisition of his system, the aphasic adult fails to suppress the more natural processes as a result of neurological disruption. this may be viewed as 'disinhibition' or 'release phenomenon'. in a sense, this may be associated with the regression hypothesis proposed by jakobson,16 and spreen.30 however, it must be stressed that this is only seen in terms of broad phonological processes and general trends, rather than specific phoneme production. the applicability of natural phonology to aphasia incidentally bears some relation to an early theorist, pick, who explained various speech disturbances on the basis of a 'disinhibition principle'.5 the writer suggested an alternative term to describe the phonological impairment in this adult aphasic s, namely: "phonological disinhibition". the word 'disinhibition' was used as it relates to the theory of natural phonology, but is quite distinct from that used by luria.21 an m/u analysis may be a useful tool for an in-depth phonological analysis, and may indeed supplement existing standardized tests. the implications extend outside clusters into all aspects of phonological impairment. it die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 98 lesley wolk may facilitate an analysis in terms of (m) and (u) segments, graded on a continuum of relative complexity values. phonological errors are assessed within a contextual framework, thus encouraging a certain amount of flexibility in relation to different phonemic environments. the relative complexity values facilitate the hierarchical arrangement of speech sounds and clusters, on which therapy can be based. it is essential to teach phonemes within a contrastive framework, yet defined within a scale of complexity as postulated in m.t. 2 1 an assessment of natural phonological processes as outlined in table vi, may provide an understanding of the 'system' and rules governing the error patterns of an individual patient. central to the concept of 'naturalness' is the discoverable "path" from input to output, within complex sound changes.6 for example, in cluster reduction, d.f. theory would require a direct 'jump' from pr p, whereas m.t provides an explanation for the entire process, possibly from pr-^ pl-^ ρ in terms of complexity values. markedness theory thus seems to provide the intermediate steps necessary for prediction of error patterns or vulnerability of a particular phoneme to change; direction of change, evaluation of progress and accurate planning of steps towards rehabilitation. with validation from further research, this approach might prove to be useful in the analysis and treatment of other phonological impairments. references 1. blumstein, s.e. (1973): a phonological investigation of aphasic speech. brown university and aphasia research centre, boston. mouton, the hague. 2. cairns, c.e. (1969): markedness, neutralization, and universal redundancy rules. language., 45, 863-885. 3. cairns, h.s. (1975): phonetic feature theory: the linguist, the speech scientist and the speech pathologist. j.comm. dis., 8, 157170. 4. chomsky, n., and halle, m. (1968): the sound pattern of english. new york, harper and row. 5. critchley, m. (1952): articulatory defects in aphasia. j. laryngoi. ο to i., 66, 1-17. 6. darden, b.j. (1974): introduction, in natural phonology — papers from the parasession on natural phonology, chicago linguistic society. april 18th. 7. deal, j.l., and darley, f.l. (1972): the influence of linguistic and situational variables on phonemic accuracy in apraxia of speech. j. speech hear. res., 15(3), 639-653. ; 8. de renzi, e., and vignolo, l.a. (1966): oral apraxia and aphasia. cortex, 2, 50-73. 9. fry, d.b. (1959): phonemic substitutions in an aphasic patient. lang speech, 2, 52-61. \ 10. goldman, r„ and fristoe, m.s. (1969): goldman-fristoe test of' articulation. american guidance service, inc., minnesota. the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) markedness analysis of phonology in aphasia 99 11. goldman, r., fristoe, m.s., and woodcock, r.w. (1970): goldmanfristoe test of auditory discrimination. american guidance service, inc., minnesota. 12. goodglass, h., and kaplan, e. (1972): the assessment of aphasia and related disorders. boston veterans administration hospital and aphasia research centre, department of neurology, boston university. 13. hatfield, f.m., and walton, k. (1975): phonological patterns in a case of aphasia. lang, and speech., 18, 341-357. 14. hyman, l.m. (1975): phonology: theory and analysis. holt, rinehart and winston, new work u.s.a. 15. ingram, d. (1976): phonological disability in children. studies in language rehabilitation and remediation 2. department of linguistics, university of british columbia. 16. jacobson, r. (1968): child language, aphasia and phonological universals. mouton, n.v. the hague. 17. johns, d.f., and darley, f.l. (1970): phonemic variability in apraxia of speech. j. speech. hear. res., 13, 556-583. 18. johnson, w„ darley, f.l., and sprietersbach, d.c. (1963): diagnostic methods in speech pathology. new york: harper and row. 19. kagan, a. (1976): a linguistic analysis of expressive and receptive phonological difficulties in apraxia, and its relevance to therapy: a case study. unpublished research report, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. 20. la pointe, l.l. and johns, d.f. (1975): some phonemic characteristics in apraxia of speech. j.comm. dis., 8, 259-269. 21. luria, a.r. (1970): traumatic aphasia, its syndromes, psychology and treatment. mouton, the hague. 22. martin, a.d. (1974): some objections to the term apraxia of speech. j. speech hear. dis., 39(1), 53-64. 23. martin, a.d., wasserman, n.h., gilden, l„ gerstman, l. and west, j. a. (1975): a process model of repetition in aphasia: an investigation of phonological and morphological interactions in aphasic error performance. brain and lang., 2, 434-450. 24. mc reynold, l.v., engmann, d., and dimmitt, k., (1974): markedness theory and articulation errors. j. speech, hear. dis., 39(1), 93-103. 25. menyuk, p. (1968): the role of distinctive features in children's acquisition of phonology. j. speech, hear. res., 11, 138-146. 26. olmsted, d.l. (1971): out of the mouth of babes. earliest stages in language learning. mouton and co. n.v., the hague. 27. rosenbek, j.c., wertz, r.t., and darley, f.l. (1973): oral sensation and perception in apraxia of speech. j. speech. hear. res., 16, 2235. 28. sarno, m.t., and levita, e. (1971): natural course of recovery in severe aphasia. arch. ofphys. med. and rehab., 52, 175-178. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 100 lesley wolk 29. shankweiler, d., and harris, k.s. (1966): an experimental approach to the problem of articulation in aphasia. cortex., 11, 277-292. 30. spreen, o. (1968): psycholinguistic aspects of aphasia. j. speech hear. res., 11(3), 467-480. 31. standel, j., gardner, j., and hannah, e.p. (1974): distinctive feature analysis. chap. 7 in applied linguistic analysis hannah, e.p. (ed.), joyce publications, california. 32. stanley, r. (1967): redundancy rules in phonology. language., 43 393-436. 33. trost, j., and canter c.j. (1974): apraxia of speech in patients with broca's aphasia: a study of phonemic production accuracy and error pattern. brain and lang., 1, 63-79. 34. walsh, h. (1974): on certain practical inadequacies of distinctive feature systems. j. speech hear. dis., 39(1), 32-43. ch rom a/son y/cctv do you use video equipment in therapy? chroma provides a service of hire and sales, of the total range of sony video and audio equipment. chroma p.o. box 31483 braamfontein 2017 johannesburg tel. 46-1750 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) less disturbance from noise improved speech discrimination more natural sound wider frequency range the lineariser is a tiny acoustic device which eliminates the worst effects of resonance caused by the sound tube of behindthe-ear hearing aids. the lineariser is fitted on the top of the earhook and can be attached to almost any hearing aid in a few seconds approximately 90% of hearing aid users report an immediate i m p r o v e m e n t with the lineariser fitted to the hearing aid. in many cases successful results are achieved with the lineariser where people have not previously been able to use a hearing aid. available from: acoustimed (pty) ltd 515 louis pasteur building schoeman st. pretoria tel: 23202 (pretoria) hearing services 315 bosman buildings cor. eloff and bree streets johannesburg tel: 372978 manufactured by: acoustimed (pty) ltd 302 sandown centre maud street, sandown tell 784-0152 agents throughout south africa die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech discrimination abilities of hearing impaired children using conventional hearing aids and radio neckloop at different signal-to-noise ratios asoka moodley m.ed (audio)(manchester) hearing impaired service, humberside county council, england abstract speech discrimination scores of 20 hearing impaired children with a mean age of 14 years were examined when using their own conventional hearing aids on the microphone setting and the radio neck loop with and without the use of the environment microphone of the radio receiver. testing of speech discrimination was administered in noise where s/n of +20db, +10db and odb were used. electroacoustic measurements of the hearing aids used were carried out to ascertain the extent to which the frequency response was altered when the aid was coupled to the neck loop used on the telecoil setting. results highlighted the excellent performance of individual hearing aids when the favourable s/n +20db was used. the use of the environment microphone on the radio receiver did not significantly affect speech discrimination scores, implications regarding the radio neck loop and the use of amplification in the classroom situation are discussed. opsomming die spraakdiskriminasietellings van 20 gehoorgestremde kinders met 'n gemiddelde ouderdom van 14 jaar is vergelyk tydens gebruik van hulle eie konvensionele gehoorapparate op die mikrofooninstelling en die radio neklussisteem met en sender gebruik van die radio ontvangsmikrofoon. spraakdiskriminasietoetsing in geraas met 'n sein tot ruisverhouding van +20db, +10db en odb is uitgevoer. ten einde die frekwensieresponsveranderinge te bepaal wanneer die gehoorapparaat, gekoppel aan die neklussisteem op die induksielusinstelling geplaas word, is elektroakoestiese metings van die gehoorapparate wat gebruik is, uitgevoer. resultate het die uitstekende werkvermoe van individuele gehoorapparate in 'n gunstige sein tot ruisverhouding van +20db beklemtoon. spraakdiskriminasietellings is nie betekenisvol geaffekteer deur die gebruik van die omgewingsmikrofoon wat aan die radio ontvanger gekoppel was. die implikasies rakende die radio neklussisteem en die gebruik van amplifikasie in die klaskamersituasie word bespreek. the importance of auditory stimulation for hearing impaired children cannot be over-emphasised. such children, particularly those with severe or profound hearing losses, need to derive maximum benefit from amplification if they are to undergo auditory training with the ultimate aim of communication. in the past, hearing losses of more than 85 db or 90 db have been considered virtually unaidable injso far as they could be expected to contribute to the recognition of speech. during the last decade, in educational situations, the writer has seen a total change in expectations. whereas children with a hearing loss of 90 db or more would have been classified as severely/profoundly deaf, and referred to schools for the deaf, nowadays, a child with a 90 db loss would be expected to function as partially hearing and often would be integrated into ordinary schools. ! one of the main problems facing any hearing aid user, and especially children who integrate into ordinary classrooms, is the varying acoustic conditions throughout the school. hearing aids amplify and possibly distort both the desired speech signals and any environmental noises having components between the effective high and low frequency cut-offs. hearing aid users have to adjust to the fact that whatever signal-to-noise ratios exist from moment to moment at the face of the microphone will also exist in their ear canals except at higher levels for both speech and noise. one of the major acoustic problems facing hearing impaired children in educational settings is noisy reverberent classrooms. (john 1957; sanders 1965; finitzo-hieber 1978; ross & giolas 1972). several studies have shown that hearing impaired listeners require a higher signal-to-noise ratio thlan normal listeners in order to achieve their maximum speech discrimination score. gengel (1971) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 reported that hearing impaired subjects rated as relatively effortless, listening conditions of -i-17 db s/n ratio for fluctuating noise and +20 db s/n ratio for constant noise. in contrast ratios less than +10 db required so much effort that the subjects indicated that under these conditions "they would prefer not to use their hearing aids but to rely on speech reading and manual communication'', (gengel 1971). nabelek and pickett (1974) report that a signal-tonoise ratio of +10 db "is the lowest ratio that should be considered for hearing impaired listeners wearing hearing aids in reverberent rooms". radio aids one of the ways of reducing the damaging effect of noise and reverberation is to ensure that the child receives the teacher's speech in the most favourable signal-to-noise ratio relationship possible. it seems reasonable to assume that if the distance between the teacher and the microphone of the child's amplifying system could be reduced, his ability to understand the teacher's speech should substantially increase. hearing aids not entirely worn on the person — more commonly referred to as radio aids, seem to meet this requirement and have been used increasingly in the united kingdom over the last 10-12 years. much of the earlier research on radio aids had been concerned with design, construction, batteries, acoustic gain, dynamic range and compression (n.c.t.d. 1976, bates and holsgrove 1979). markides, huntingdon and kettlety (1980) compared a range of hearing aids including infra red systems, radio aids and speech training units. markides et al. reported disappointment with radio aids as they "did not fulfil their theoretical potential". however, the authors did note that in poor listening conditions, radio aids have much to © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 asoka moodley be said for them. in recent years there has been a sharp increase in the use of personal f.m. systems. the "radio received" signal picked up by the child's radio receiver is passed on to his hearing aid for amplification in one of two ways: (i) by direct connection (audio input), (ii) by loop induction which involves the child wearing the loop of cord around his neck and under his clothes (neck loop). it is mainly to neck loops that this study is directed. in the neck loop system the child sets his hearing aids to the telecoil at "mt" position. the electrical signal from the f.m. receiver is routed to the small loop of cord worn around the neck. this neck loop emits an electromagnetic field that is picked up by the telecoil of the child's hearing aid, amplified by the aid, transduced by its receiver and delivered via the earmould to the child's ear. if the child's hearing aid does not have the "mt" facility, it is possible for him to use a receiver which has an inbuilt environmental microphone. both the radio neck loop and direct audio input systems are very attractive to schools and students in further education and there is now widespread use of these systems throughout the united kingdom. however, very little is known about the ways in which incorporation of the personal hearing aid into the direct audio input or neck loop fm system alters the characteristics of the sound delivered to the child's ear and what effect these systems have on the discrimination of speech in various listening conditions. hawkins and van tasell (1982) reported that the frequency response of the hearing aid altered when used with the neck loop and they went on to suggest that "the altered frequency response due to the fm neck loop configuration can be expected to affect speech perception differently for each hearing impaired child". they found that the frequency response curves for post-aural aids in the microphone and direct audio input modes are virtually identical being ±2 db from 250-6300 hz. most investigators agree that the frequency response of a hearing aid inductance loop system is frequently different from that of the hearing aid operating on the microphone input. however there is some conflicting data on the nature of this difference. hodgson and sung (1971) report relatively greater gain in frequencies below lkhz when the hearing aid is operating via the telecoil mode. nolan (1982), on the other hand, looked at various hearing aids and was very critical of the low frequency amplification that most aids afforded on the telecoil setting. his findings are supported by barr-hamilton (1978), matkin and oslen (1970) and huntingdon (1976) whose studies show that the low frequency gain dropped sharply when the hearing aid was switched from the microphone to the telecoil position. sung, sung and hodgson (1973) noted fluctuations of between 8 db to 30 db when a hearing aid was switched from microphone to telecoil setting. kortschot (cited by b0rrild (1968)) reported that hearing impaired children showed significantly better speech discrimination scores when using the inductance loop than with conventional hearing aids. on the contrary, vargo et al (1970), found that speech signals were significantly less intelligible when the hearing aid operated on the telecoil setting than on the microphone setting. one factor that is common to all the studies, and something about which there is little disagreement, is that changes do take place in the frequency characteristics of hearing aids when they are switched from the microphone setting to the telecoil setting. method aims of the study the present study was undertaken with four aims in mind: 1. to examine the speech discrimination abilities of hearing impaired children using their conventional hearing aids and radio neck loop in conditions where signal-to-noise ratios of +20db, +10db and odb were used. 2. to find out whether there was any significant difference in the speech discrimination scores of children when use was made of the environmental microphone on the radio receiver. 3. to examine and compare the electroacoustic characteristics of the aid set on the telecoil position coupled to the radio neck loop and the microphone setting of the aid. 4. to ascertain the noise levels generated in the hearing aid on the telecoil setting compared to the microphone setting. subjects twenty hearing impaired children of average age 14 years 0 months, with an age range of 10 years 10 months 16 years 6 months, took part in this study. the average hearing loss in the better ear averaged across the frequencies 250hz-4khz, was in the range 47 db to 88 db. all the children had sensori-neural hearing losses and the onset of deafness was at birth or in the first eighteen months of life. all the children were regular hearing aid users. eighteen of the children wore binaural post-aural aids and two of the children were monaural users of post-aural aids. relevant particulars of the children who took part in the study are shown in tables 1 and 2. table 1 number, age, sex and hearing levels of children taking part sex no. mean age in years range hearing levels in db average 250-4khz in the better ear. mean range boys 12 13.5 10.10-14.10 72.0 49 -87 girls 8 14.5 13.11-16. 6 67.7 44.5-87 boys & girls 20 14.0 10.10-16. 6 69.35 47 -88 i table 2 hearing aids used by the children all children wore post-aural aids 18 were binaural users 2 were monaural users types of hearing aid used n.h.s. b.e.51 = 6 n.h.s. b.e.ll = 4 philips hp8276 = 4 siemens pp.agc = 2 widex = 1 oticon e22p = 1 philips hp8273 = 1 phonak pp-c = 1 20 / physical arrangement of the room and equipment the tests were carried out in an acoustically treated room measuring 18 feet by 14 feet. the floors were fully carpeted, the windows were curtained, and the walls and ceilings were covered with acoustically treated tiles. the conditions were as ideal as could be expect/ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech discrimination abilities of hearing-impaired children using conventional hearing aids 7 9 ed. the reverberation time was within acceptable limits (approx. 0,5 sec.). 5' radio transmitter mic. 6 " speech 14' figure 1 physical arrangement of the free-field speech discrimination test. figure 1 shows the physical arrangements of the free-field speech discrimination test. the child was seated 5 feet from the speaker delivering the speech. the transmitter/microphone was supported by a chemical stand 6" from the spjeaker in order to simulate classroom-listening conditions. two speakers delivering the wide-band noise were positioned at a 45° angle to each ear of the child and placed five feet away, on a level plane with the ear. 1 the a.b. word lists were recorded on tape and played through a tandberg 6000x tape recorder via a jessops speech audiometer attachment (s. a. a.) number 420 terminating in a chartweil ls3/5a speaker. competing noise, which was white noise shaped as cocktail party noise, was also recorded on tape and played through a uher type 4000l tape recorder via a jessop speech audiometer attachment (saa) number 420 terminating in two chartweil ls3/5a speakers. both speech audiometer attachments were calibrated at 5 db intervals. the speech signal was always presented at the same intensity level of 130 db (saa) dial reading. the intensity of the competing noise (using the saa reading) was varied accordingly to obtain signal-to-noise ratios of +20db, +10db and odb. at the beginning of both the speech and the noise tapes a 1 000 hz pure tone was recorded for calibration purposes (0 db vu meter deflection on the saa). the 130 db dial reading for speech was equivalent to speech peaking at 70 dba, and the 125 db dial reading for noise was equivalent to 70 dba. the measurement was carried out with a β. & k. sound level meter type 2203 (linear mode) at 5 feet distance from the speakers at the position where the child was seated. procedure speech discrimination testing each subject was tested separately using his own hearing aid on the microphone setting, using the radio neck loop with the environmental microphone, and using the radio neck loop without the environmental microphone. subjects were tested in three signal-to-noise ratios for each mode of listening: s/n +20db; s/n +10db; s/n odb. each subject was asked to listen carefully to speech prior to each listening condition (a few a.b. words were given) and to adjust the volume control of his hearing aid to his/her most comfortable listening level. the subject was asked to select at random a card on to which the three modes of listening, the three signal-to-noise ratios, and the word list for each listening condition were indicated in randomized order. each child was then instructed as follows: you are going to hear some words. you will also hear some hissing noise from the speakers behind you. listen carefiilly to the words and repeat each word whatever you think you heard. even if you hear part of a word or a word that does not make sense, or even a single sound like 'p' or y please repeat it. children were asked to switch from using their hearing aid microphone, to using the neck loop with their hearing aids set to the telecoil position, according to the cards they selected. the environmental microphone of the radio receiver was switched on and off according to the card they had chosen. a careful note was made of the volume setting of the hearing aids and the radio receiver as these were to be used in electroacoustic measurements after the speech discrimination test. the responses of each child for each listening condition and at each signal-to-noise ratio were recorded on specially designed forms and later on, scored, based on the number of phonemes correctly repeated. electroacoustic measurements electroacoustic measurements were carried out on the child's hearing aid using the phonic ear hc1000 acoustic test computer. (i) the hearing aid was tested as an ordinary aid on the microphone setting at the child's usual volume setting. the internal noise of the hearing aid in this mode was measured by pressing the test push button on the test box. this cuts out the signal going to the hearing aid microphone and thus a measure of the noise being generated by the hearing aid is obtained, (ii) measurements were then carried out with the child's hearing aid set to the telecoil position and linked to the radio receiver. the hearing aid was coupled to the 2 cm3 coupler of the test box and placed on a tailor's dummy at a position closely approximating the way the hearing aid would normally be worn by the child. the neck loop was placed around the neck of the dummy. the internal noise of the hearing aid at the telecoil setting was then measured in the manner described above. a record of all the electroacoustic measurements was made on specially designed forms and graphs were plotted. as a final measure, the hearing aid set on the telecoil position was moved around gradually within the neckloop and observations were noted of any fluctuations in output of the aid as it was positioned in different parts, within the magnetic field created by the neck loop. after the whole sample had been tested, one third of the sample was chosen at random and· re-tested on all the measures. t die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 asoka moodley results speech discrimination scores a comparison of the mean speech discrimination scores achieved with the conventional hearing aid on microphone setting and the radio neck loop at three signal-to-noise ratios is shown in table 3. all this information is shown graphically in fig. 2. the results were analysed using "t" tests of significance to find out how the different hearing aids performed under conditions of noise. table 3 comparison of mean speech discrimination scores (% phonemes) of hearing aid microphone and radio neck loop at 3 different s/n ratios. faired t test values and levels of significance are given s/n ratios hearing aid microphone radio neck loop plus env. mic. radio neck loop minus env. mic. s/n +20db 74,8% 72,3% t = 0,62 n/s at 0,05 or 0,01 level 66,5% t = 2,08 significant at 0,05 level s/n +10db 57,6% 74,3% t = 3,8 significant at 0,05 and 0,01 level 68,1% t = 2,62 significant at 0,05 and 0,01 level s/n odb 36,1% 63,5% t = 4,85 significant at 0,05 and 0,01 level 63,5% t = 4,82 significant at 0,05 and 0,01 level s/n +20db the most favourable signal-to-noise ratio was +20db when the conventional hearing aid was on its microphone setting producing a mean speech discrimination score of 74,8% and 72,3% when the radio neck loop was used with or without the environmental microphone of the radio receiver. 1 50 ) — ) / / x = he arint x k ll ck u aid oop v >op u mil. (itii ithol in vir jt en dnme yirols ntal m e n ! mic. al μ 1c. c ο = ni = ne arint x k ll ck u aid oop v >op u mil. (itii ithol in vir jt en dnme yirols ntal m e n ! mic. al μ 1c. τ 1 , +20 figure 2 mean speech discrimination scores for individual hearing aids and radio neckloop. s/n +10db at a signal-to-noise ratio of +10db, the performance of the hearing aid falls 17,2% while the performance of the neck loop remained much the same. at this signal-to-noise ratio there is a significant difference between the performance of the conventional hearing aid when compared to the performance of the neck loop, with and without the environmental microphone (p>0.01). s/n odb the most unfavourable signal-to-noise ratio was odb, when a marked deterioration in the performance of the conventional hearing aid on its microphone setting was noted. this represents a fall of 38,7% when considering the performance at s/n +20db. there is a significant difference between the performance of the conventional hearing aid and the radio neck loop (p>0.01). these results show that background noise affects both types of hearing aids but that the conventional hearing aid on its microphone setting is more adversely affected by noisy conditions. as the noise level increases the performance of the conventional hearing aid rapidly deteriorates (see figure 2). use of the environmental microphone on radio receiver paired "t" tests of significance were undertaken to find out what effect, if any, the environmental microphone had on the performance of the radio neck loop at the three signal-to-noise ratios used in the study. table 4 illustrates the results and shows levels of significance. i table 4 comparison of mean speech discrimination scores (% phonemes) of radio neck loop with and without use of the environmental microphone at 3 different s/n ratios. paired t test values and levels of significance are given. | s/n ratios radio neck loop with env. microphone radio neck loop without 1 env. microphone s/n +20db 72,3% 66,5% t = 1,3 n/s s/n +10db 74,3% 68,1% t = 1,85 ' n/s s/n odb 63,5% 63,5% x = 0 . ' ' ' n/s when the performance of the neck loop with environmental microphone was compared to its performance without the environmental microphone at similar signal-to-noise ratios no significant differences were found. in other words, at similar signal-to-noise ratios, / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s p e e c h discrimination abilities of hearing-impaired children using conventional hearing aids table 5 test-retest-reliability of speech discrimination scores mean phoneme discrimination in % 81 hearing aid mic. radio neckloop + env. mic. radio neckloop — env. mic. test s.d. retest s.d. r test s.d. retest s.d. r test s.d. retest s.d. r signal-to-noise ratio in db. +20db 75.5 14.0 74.9 11.9 .94 74.9 15.1 77.1 13.8 .96 74.9 9.5 74.4 6.57 .85 +10db 60.5 20.1 66.6 17.7 .82 79.3 10.4 77.2 10.4 .95 71.65 9.7 72.1 9.48 .84 odb 38.85 26.2 47.15 20.1 .75 61.5 16.9 61.0 16.0 .92 66.1 21.8 66.5 15.6 .97 the use of the environmental microphone did not significantly affect discrimination for speech. on the other hand, the use of the environmental microphone at s/n +20db compared to the use of the environmental microphone at s/n odb produced a significant difference at the 0.05 level of confidence. this appears to indicate that while the environmental microphone has no adverse effect on speech discrimination in reasonable levels of background noise, it should not be used in extremely noisy conditions, especially when the source of the noise is close to the environmental microphone. test — retest reliability of speech discrimination scores the test retest reliability of the speech discrimination scores was ascertained by retesting a third of the sample chosen at random: the mean speech discrimination scores of the children in both the test and retest reliability sessions are shown in table 5. the values of the pearson coefficient of correlation (r) between the original and retest scores of the children are also shown. the mean scores of the children in both the test and retest sessions were very similar indicating a high degree of reliability. electroacoustic measurements large differences were noted between the performance of the hearing aid on its microphone setting as compared to the performance of the hearing aid when used in the "t" position, in conjunction with the heck loop. in nearly all cases the performance of the aid with the neck loop was inferior to its performance on the microphone setting. similar hearing aids at identical volume settings produced frequency response curves on the telecoil setting which were^ sometimes vastly different. i internal noise generated by the| hearing aids noise levels generated in all the hearing aids used in conjunction with the neck loop were much higher than noise levels generated in the hearing aids when used on the microphone setting. it is clear from the results that the transduction of the signal by the loop causes an increase in the internal noise that reaches the users eardrum. table 6 shows the noise levels generated by hearing aids when used as an ordinary aid and when the aid is used with the neck loop. , 2 , ^ • _ ^ k \ ν 3 , ' ιικλκγ = telec 1g aid ογ oil sett • ιικλκγ = telec 1g aid ογ oil sett ing 0 250 500 ik 2k 3k 4k 5k frequencies in hz figure 3 b.e. volume 3 rec. vol. 4.5 hearing aid set on "t" position and coupled with neckloop held at three different positions in relation to the neckloop. the effect of distance of the hearing aid from the neck loop it was found that by moving the hearing aid arbitrarily at different positions within the neck loop there was a great fluctuation in the output of the hearing aid. the effect of distance of a hearing aid from the neck loop is shown in fig. 3 where aid b.e.11 is given as an example. numbers 1, 2 and 3 show the three different positions the hearing aid was held at in relation to the neck loop. these large differences in the output of the hearing aid could well result when children tilt their heads, or bend forwards or backwards in the course of the day. in addition, average gain will vary from child to child depending on neck lengths (hawkins and van tasell 1982). constantly fluctuating gain due to the child's head movements could be very distracting to a child and could well affect his ability to perceive speech adequately. discussion most workers in the field of audiology agree that the use of speech discrimination tests both in quiet and noise is the most adequate approach to the evaluation of hearing aids. in the present study there was no statistically significant difference between conventional hearing aids normally worn by the children, and the radio neck loop at a signal-to-noise ratio +20db, that is, under good acoustic conditions. the differences only became apparent under conditions of noise. there is no doubt that individual post-aural hearing aids, when they have been correctly prescribed, are working at maximum efficientable 6 internal noise levels (shown in db) generated in the various types of hearing aids used in the study. type of noise noise difference hearing aid generated generated ' in noise on telecoil on mic. levels setting setting b.e.51 108 88 20 b.e.i 1 88 68 20 widex ao pp 78 68 10 otikon e22p 98 78 20 phonak super front 88 78 10 philips hp8273 92 85 7 siemans 24pp-agc 88 | 78 10 philips hp8276 76 56 20 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 asoka moodley cy, and are used in good acoustic conditions have much to offer. their most positive advantage is that they are small and hence more cosmetically acceptable. they can be worn throughout the day both in school and after school. they are more readily acceptable particularly to teenagers because they can easily be concealed. the results of this study highlighted their excellent performance under favourable acoustic conditions. at a s/n of +20db, their performance surpassed that of the radio neck loop, though the difference in mean discrimination scores did not approach statistical significance. however.under poor acoustic conditions such as noisy classrooms and at a distance from the speaker, individual hearing aids rapidly lose their effectiveness. this study has shown that at a signal-to-noise ratio of +10db the performance of the individual aids falls from 74,8% to 57,6% and at a signal-to-noise ratio of odb the performance of the individual aids falls a further 21,5%. thus there is an overall deterioration of over 38 % in poor acoustic conditions. in many classroom conditions, the distance between the nearest desk and the blackboard (where the teacher would normally stand) is rarely less than six feet. at such distances a pupil using an individual hearing aid would lose much of the information which would be constantly buried in background noise. most ordinary classrooms have high levels of background noise. this combined with distance from the teacher make individual hearing aids almost useless in such classrooms. in recent years with accumulating evidence on the detrimental effects of noise and reverberation on speech perception, there has been widespread use of radio aids. this study confirms the need for some, type of radio system in the education of hearing impaired children. the more recent radio aids which incorporate the use of the child's individual hearing aid have become increasingly popular and at the moment seem to provide the answer to noise and reverberation problems. an examination of the results of this study shows that the radio neck loop certainly has advantages over the individual hearing aid in conditions of noise. at a signal-to-noise ratio of odb (the most unfavourable signal-to-noise ratio in this study) the performance of the individual aids falls by 38,7%, while the performance of the radio neck loop falls to under 10%. furthermore the use of the environmental microphone does not adversely affect speech discrimination scores at any of the signal-to-noise ratios in this investigation. this is an important consideration as it is vital for children to be able to hear their own voices and environmental sounds. it is important to bear in mind that the scores achieved with the use of the neck loop in this study relate to a group of children who normally make extensive use of the inductance loop at school and have been doing so for several years now. the children use the telecoil facility of their hearing aids to watch television. they use it in the hall and chapel almost daily and most of the classrooms are looped. these children then,,have extensive experience of perceiving speech through the telecoil facility of their hearing aids. young, inexperienced hearing aid users who have not been exposed to similar experience of using the telecoil facility of their hearing aids might 'not be in a similar position and may suffer a loss in speech discrimination resulting from the changes in the frequency response of the hearing aid which take place. secondly, during the test situation, children sat upright with the neck loop carefully positioned around their necks and there was little likelihood of fluctuation in the strength of the speech signal. this is not the case in the day-to-day use of the radio neck loop in a classroom where children are moving around and moving their heads from side to side. the teacher cannot assume that the child using the radio neck loop is receiving the same signal as when the individual hearing aid is worn in the microphone mode. all the hearing aids used in the study showed that when the hearing aid was used in conjunction with the neck loop, there was a change in the frequency response of the hearing aid. in most cases the greatest change occurred in the lower region of the frequency range. this would therefore have serious limitations for profoundly hearing impaired children who depend almost entirely on lower frequencies for their information. the study also showed that the location of the hearing aid in relation to the neck loop is a critical variable and a matter of great concern to users of the neck loop. the great fluctuation in the output of the hearing aid as it is moved around within the neck loop can cause the child to lose audibility of the signal as he moves his head about. older children who are experienced hearing aid users, and those who have less severe hearing impairments, may be sensitive to this and make the necessary adjustments. young, inexperienced hearing aid users are less discriminating in their use of hearing aids and will suffer a loss in perception of the speech signal. the general implication resulting from this study is that more attention should be given to the performance characteristics of hearing aids used in conjunction with the radio neckloop. not only do different hearing aids show different sensitivity for loop induction, but when hearing aids are chosen for their performance on the microphone setting, these advantages are lost when the hearing aid is coupled to the radio neck loop and has to be used on the telecoil setting of the aid. in view of the number of limitations that the radio neck loop has, it should not be the radio system of choice when considering the provision of radio systems for children. what is required is a system that offers all the advantages of reducing noise and reverberation and yet preserves the frequency response of the hearing aid that has been prescribed for the child. the performance of a radio microphone hearing aid system should be equivalent on both audio and radio channels. this means that a child does not suffer from the change that takes place in the frequency response of the aid when it is switched from the microphone to the telecoil setting. direct audio-input seems to be the answer to this problem. in the study by hawkins and van tasell (1982) it was shown that hearing aids showed a remarkable similarity in frequency response when used with the direct audio input facility, in contrast to the, variable and often unpredictable, microphone-telecoil differences. there is a wide range of hearing aids on the market today and there is more flexibility in the way they can be adjusted to cater for a variety of different hearing losses. individual aids can therefore be chosen and set as far as possible to cater for the special needs of each child's hearing loss. as hearing aids are prescribed largely on the basis of their microphone performance it would make sense to preserve this frequency response while at the same time offering the child the benefits of reduced noise and reverberation that audio input would provide. in addition, the direct audio input system is not susceptible to the effects of distance and hearing aid orientation that has been noticed with the aid coupled to the radio neck loop. thus a child whose hearing aid was specifically prescribed because of the low frequency emphasis which it afforded him would not be at a disadvantage in this respect when the aid was used in conjunction with the audio input facility. acknowledgements the author wishes to express his thanks to: the children at ovingdean hall school in brighton where much of this study was underthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech discrimination abilities of hearing-impaired children using conventional hearing aids 83 taken; dr michael nolan of the department of audiology, manchester university for his advice and assistance with this study; and mr r. bennett, county hall, beverley for his assistance with the illustrations. references barr hamilton, r.m.a. a theoretical approach to the loop induction system. brit. j. audio., 12, 135-139, 1978. bates, a. and holsgrove, g. the phonic ear. teacher of the deaf, (3), 5, 154-170, 1979. b6rrild, k. the induction loop and its possibilities. in g. liden (ed.) geriatric audiology, almquist and wiksell, stockholm, 1968. finitzo-hieber, τ., and tillman, t.w. room acoustics effect on monosyllabic word discrimination ability for normal and hearing impaired children. j. speech hear. res., 21, 440-458, 1978. gengel, r.w. acceptable speech-to-noise ratios for aided speech discrimination by the hearing impaired. j. audio. res., 11, 219-222, 1971. hawkins, d.b., and van tasell, d.j. electroacoustic characteristics of personal f.m. systems. j. speech hear. disord., 47, 355-362, 1982. hodgson, w.r., and sung, r.j. performance of individual hearing aids utilizing microphone and induction coil input. j. speech hear. res., 14, 365-371, 1971. huntingdon, a. tests on induction loops in current use in schools for the deaf. teacher of the deaf, 74, 7-18, 1976. john, j.e.j. acoustics in the use of hearing aids: educational guidance and the deaf child. manchester university press, 1957. markides a. huntingdon, α., and kettlety, a. comparison of speech discrimination abilities through infra red radio and conventional hearing aids. teacher of the deaf, (4), 5-14, 1980. matkin, n. and oslen, w. response of hearing aids with induction loop amplification systems, amer. annals deaf, 115, 73-78, 1970. nabelek, a.k., and pickett, j.m. monaural and binaural speech perception through hearing aids under noise and reverberation with normal and hearing impaired listeners. j. speech hear. res., 17, 724-739, 1974. national council for teachers of the deaf. the phonic earhc421 fm stereo hearing aid. northern branch hearing aid subcommittee report. national council for teachers of the deaf. u.k., 1976. nolan, m. hearing aids for children. teacher of the deaf, (6) 5, 122, 1982. ross, m., and giolas, t.g. audiology management of hearing lmpaird children: principles and prerequisites for intervention, university park press, baltimore, 1980. sanders, d. noise conditions in normal school classrooms excep. children, 31, 344-551, 1965. sung, r.j. sung, g.s., and hodgson, w.r. telecoil versus microphone perforrrgnce in hearing aids. volta review, 19, 15, 417-424, 1973. vargo, s.w., taylor g., and tannahill, j.c. the intelligibility of speech by hearing aids on induction loop and microphone modes of signal reception. j. speech hear. res., 13, 87-91,1970. address for reprints: t.a. moodley, hearing impaired service, lowfield lane, melton, n. ferriby, n. humberside, hu14 3ht, england. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aids for plus the development of perception the acquisition of speech and language skills the improvement of motor co-ordination helpful texts for therapists educational toys, books and equipment records for auditory training catalogues on request large variety of tests available stockists of learning to listen two sound lottos full lda range ο play and schoolroom 44 president place, 148 jan smuts ave., rosebank. telephone 788-1304 p.o. box 47288, parklands 2121. x the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, or critically evaluative theoretical, or therapeutic issues dealing with disorders of speech, voice hearing or language, or on aspects of the processes underlying these. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. all contributions are reviewed by at least two consultants who are not provided with author identification. form of manuscript. authors should submit four neatly typewritten manuscripts in triple spacing with wide margins which should not exceed much more than 25 pages. each page should be numbered. the first page of two copies should contain the title of the article, name of author/s, highest degree and address or institutional affiliation. the first page of the remaining two copies should contain only the title of the article. the second page of all copies should contain only an abstract (100 words) which should be provided in both english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors. all paragraphs should start at the left margin and not be indented. major headings, where applicable, should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements and references. tables and figures should be prepared on separate sheets (one per table/figure). figures, graphs and line drawings must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper. lettering "appearing on these should be uniform and professionally done, bearing in mind that such lettering should be legible after a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should not be included in the illustration but should appear below it. the titles of tables and figures should be concise but explanatory. the title of tables appears above, and of figures below. tables and figures should be numbered in order of appearance (with arabic numerals). the amount of tabular and illustrative material allowed will be at the discretion of the editor (usually not more than 6). references. references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and date, e.g. van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, et al. after the first author will suffice. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list. references should be listed alphabetically in triple-spacing at the end of the article. for acceptable abbreviations of names of journals, consult the fourth issue (october) of dsh abstracts or the world list of scientific periodicals. the number of references used should not exceed much more than 20. note the following examples: locke, j.l. clinical phonology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology, 3rd ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. proofs. galley proofs will be sent to the author wherever possible. corrections other than typographical errors will be charged to the author. reprints. 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. all manuscripts and correspondence should be addressed to: the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech and hearing association, p.o. box 31782, braamfontein 2017, south africa. inligting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer verslae en artikels oor navorsing, of krities evaluerende artikels oor die teoretiese of terapeutiese aspekte van spraak-, stem-, gehoorof taalafwykings, of oor aspekte van die prosesse onderliggend aan hierdie afwy kings. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings sal nie materiaal aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. alle bydraes word deur minstens twee konsultante nagegaan wat nie ingelig is oor die identiteit van die skrywer nie. formaat van die manuskrip. skrywers moet vier netjies getikte manuskripte in 3-spasiering en met bree kantlyn indien, en dit moet nie veel langer as 25 bladsye wees nie. elke bladsy moet genommer wees. | op die eerste bladsy van 2 afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, die naam van die skrywer/s, die hoogste graad behaal en die adres of naam van hulle betrokke instansie verskyn. op die eerste bladsy van die oorblywende twee afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskyn. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. alle paragrawe moet teenaan die linkerkantlyn begin word en moet nie ingekeep word nie. hoofopskrifte moet, waar dit van toepassing is, in die volgende volgorde wees: metode, resultate, bespreking, gevolgtrekking, erkennings en verwysings. tabelle en figure moet op afsonderlike bladsye verskyn (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie). figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge moet oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte gedoen word. letterwerk wat hierop verskyn moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50%-verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by die illustrasie moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. de suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 verklarings of omskrywings moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maar daaronder verskyn. die byskrifte van tabelle moet bo-aan verskyn en die van figure onderaan. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn genommer word (met arabiese syfers). die hoeveelheid materiaal in die vorm van tabelle en illustrasies wat toegelaat word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). verwysings. verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum bv. van riper (1979). waar daar meer as twee skrywers is, sal et al. na die eerste skrywer voldoende wees. die name van alle skrywers moet in die \ferwysingslys verskyn. verwysings moet alfabeties in 3-spasiering aan die einde van die artikel gerangskik word. vir die aanvaarde afkortings van tydskrifte se titels, raadpleeg die vierde uitgawe (oktober) van dsh abstracts of the hbrld list of scientific periodicals. die getal verwysings wat gebruik is, moet nie veel meer as 20 wees nie. let op die volgende voorbeelde: locke, j.l. clinical phonology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disoiders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology, 3de ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. proewe. galeiproewe sal waar moontlik aan die skrywer gestuur word. die onkoste van veranderinge, behalwe tipografiese foute, sal deur die skrywer self gedra moet word. herdrukke. 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis verskaf word. alle manuskripte en korrespondensie moet gerig word aan: die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings. die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraaken gehoorheelkunde, posbus 31782, braamfontein 2017, suid-afrika. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pg6-12.html the translation of the vertigo symptom scale into afrikaans: a pilot study christine rogers jacques de wet ayanda gina ladine louw musa makhoba lee tacon division of communication sciences and disorders, department of health and rehabilitation sciences, university of cape town correspondence to: c rogers (christine.rogers@uct.ac.za) abstract dizziness is a common clinical problem that is challenging to diagnose and treat. one of a subset of symptoms that fall under the encompassing term of dizziness is vertigo, which is the subjective experience of hallucination of movement, often associated with vestibular disorders. while dizziness has a broad range of causes, the association between vestibular disturbance, and its attendant vertigo, and anxiety is well established. the vertigo symptom scale (vss) is a questionnaire that assesses aspects of vertigo and vertigo-related anxiety. the aim of this study was twofold. in phase 1, a translation of the vss into afrikaans was evaluated using the delphi consensus technique and two panels of participants. panel 1 comprised first-language afrikaans speakers who commented on the language, grammar and vocabulary of the items. panel 2 were bilingual health care practitioners with either a psychology background or a special interest in vertigo. after two rounds of consultation, consensus was achieved and the final translation of the afrikaans vertigo symptom scale (avss) was agreed upon, as well as a list of afrikaans words descriptive of vertigo. phase 2 used a descriptive, correlational design. the aim was to pilot the avss with a sample of vertiginous and control participants to establish its ability to differentiate between the two groups and to explore experiences of vertigo and anxiety within the two embedded subscales. the results of the pilot study yielded significant statistical differences (p<0.001) between the groups on both subscales of the tool. preliminary results suggest that the avss is able to identify patients with vertiginous disturbance and anxiety. the avss presents with good sensitivity and specificity as measured by the receiver-operating characteristic (roc) curve. afrikaans is the home language of almost 6 million people in south africa. the translation of the vss into afrikaans presents health care professionals with a tool with which to assess vertigo and vertigo-related anxiety in this population. keywords: afrikaans, anxiety, delphi, dizziness, vertigo, vertigo symptom scale, vestibular disorders diagnosis and management of dizziness is challenging and often a source of frustration for the clinician (kerr, 2005). reasons for this difficulty include the subjective nature of the complaints, issues that patients have when trying to describe symptoms, and the broad range of causes, ranging from vestibular to psychological, which could be responsible for the presence of symptoms. dizziness is a general term often used by patients to describe a variety of sensations, which include light-headedness, presyncope and other experiences including vertigo. vertigo is a perception of motion when there is no external source for that sensation (yardley, luxon & haacke, 1994), and is classically associated with vestibular disorders. vertigo may be viewed as a distinct clinical entity along a continuum of symptoms that are broadly described as dizziness. the identification of the presence of vertigo, and its association with vestibular dysfunction, may direct professionals such as otologists and audiologists in their choice of investigations, management and subsequent referrals. for example, it would be mandatory to conduct an audiological assessment in cases of vertigo, while dizziness related to presyncope or cardiac causes would not necessitate such tests. although an appreciation of the difference between dizziness and vertigo is critical for effective diagnosis and treatment, the two terms are often used interchangeably and are confused by both patients and clinicians (mcpherson & whitaker, 2001). attacks of vertigo may be distressing because of the associated autonomic and vegetative symptoms (mégnigbêto, sauvage & launois, 2001). in addition, vertiginous episodes have been associated with anxiety, panic and social phobia (aslan, ceylan, kemaloglu & goksu, 2003; tschan et al., 2008; wiltink et al., 2009). there is increasing evidence of an association between vestibular disorders and activation of areas of the brain concerned with emotion, and in turn with the autonomic nervous system (best et al., 2006; meli, zimatore, badaracco, de angelis & tufarelli, 2007; wiltink et al., 2009). an additional concern regarding the psychological sequelae of vestibular syndromes is that negative or maladaptive coping mechanisms, which include avoidance of stimulation, may reduce compliance with vestibular rehabilitation therapy and ultimately delay recovery (aslan et al., 2003; cohen & kimball, 2003; luxon, 2004; meli et al., 2007). it is therefore suggested that the dual elements of vestibular symptoms and potential psychological involvement are investigated in all patients who present with vertigo. a detailed, systematic and holistic case history, conducted with insight and an empathic manner, is crucial in every case and most likely to result in a diagnosis of the cause of vertigo (bennett, 2008). reliance on sophisticated clinical or laboratory testing in place of, or as an adjunct to, the case history will seldom return a useful diagnosis; indeed, it is the exception that results of formal vestibular tests would influence, or change, management decisions (shepard, 2007). while an accurate case history is essential for effective treatment, anamnesis may be more problematic when there are linguistic and cultural differences between the health care professional and patient, a scenario common in a diverse country such as south africa. a variety of questionnaires have been used in clinical practice in order to identify or assess vertigo symptoms or handicap, as well as related issues such as anxiety. one advantage of questionnaires is to focus patients’ thoughts on their complaints prior to the consultation, allowing the clinician to highlight relevant issues. questionnaires may be used as an entrée to explore areas that may otherwise be difficult to address, for example, probing symptoms of panic or anxiety may elicit a guarded or defensive response. skilfully selected questionnaires add to the completeness of the case history, and results may signal the need for further investigations or referrals. one such questionnaire, the vertigo symptom scale (vss) (yardley, masson, verschuur, haacke, & luxon, 1992), has two embedded subscales; one evaluates vertigo severity and the explores symptoms of somatic anxiety. it was developed after extensive interviews which explored the experience of patients living with vertigo. results from the vss were correlated with independent scales of anxiety and vertigo handicap, as well as diagnostic classification systems and objective testing. the resulting vss has been researched extensively and good reliability and concurrent validity have been established (yardley et al., 1992). the vss is the self-assessment scale targeting vestibular symptoms most used in clinical practice (faag, bergenius, forsberg & langius-eklöf, 2007). it has been translated into a number of languages without losing validity (tschan et al., 2008). the south african context in south africa, almost 6 million people use afrikaans as their home language, making it the third most common language spoken. a further 15 million people are proficient in afrikaans (south africa info, 2001). in the western cape, where this study was conducted, the majority (55.3%) of the population speak afrikaans (statistics south africa, 2004). english language questionnaires exploring health-related quality of life, functional assessments of chronic illness therapy and measures of mental health have been translated into afrikaans using a variety of methods (jelsma & ferguson, 2004; harpham et al., 2003; webster, cella & yost, 2003). efforts have been made to translate audiological materials, such as stimuli for speech recognition, into zulu (panday, kathard, pillay & govender, 2007). while many english language questionnaires are available for exploring aspects of dizziness and vertigo, to the authors’ knowledge none has been translated into other languages spoken in south africa. at present there are no questionnaires available in afrikaans with which to explore the experience of symptoms of vertiginous patients presenting to health care services. given the large numbers of afrikaans speakers nationally and their predominance in the western cape, a self-assessment scale was selected for translation and validation. the vss was selected for this research because of its ability to explore vertiginous symptoms and those of anxiety and panic simultaneously. delphi consensus procedure the delphi consensus procedure is a method often used in health-related research and involves obtaining consensus of opinion from knowledgeable participants through the use of structured questioning in a series of rounds (hasson, keeney, & mckenna, 2000). results from the first round of questions are relayed back to participants in subsequent rounds (de villiers, de villiers & kent, 2005) and in this study suggestions of afrikaans words that would capture the essence of the word vertigo were also sought. delphi procedures are cost-effective methods of gathering information, and in contrast to focus groups, participants are not in contact with each other, or aware of the identities of other panel members (hardy et al., 2004; powell, 2002). use of delphi consensus procedures in audiology is ongoing, with panels currently employing this method to aid the development of international classification of functioning (icf) health core sets for hearing loss (danermark et al., 2010) and vertigo (podlipny, personal communication, 10 october 2010). the aims of this study were to validate a translation of the vss (yardley et al., 1992) into afrikaans, and to conduct a pilot study of the translation’s ability to differentiate between participants with and without complaints of vertigo. the study was conducted in two phases, which will be presented in the ‘method’ section. method study design in phase 1, a delphi consensus procedure was used first, to gain agreement on the translation of the vss from two panels of participants. panel 1 comprised first-language afrikaans speakers, who commented on grammar and vocabulary used. panel 2 were bilingual health care professionals who had experience in treating patients with vestibular disorders. in addition both panels were asked to contribute afrikaans words that captured the essence of the experience of vertigo. in phase 2, a descriptive, correlational design was used and the afrikaans vertigo symptom scale (avss) was piloted among a sample of participants with and without vertigo. phase 1: translation of the vss and delphi consensus procedure the vss was translated from english into afrikaans using the steps depicted in the flow chart (figure 1). the delphi procedure was used to obtain consensus on the translation. conventional delphi designs have four rounds, but this was modified to two rounds as consensus was only sought on the translation of a pre-exisiting, validated scale. phase 1 participants two panels were selected through purposive sampling. panel 1 consisted of 5 first-language afrikaans-speaking lay participants, and panel 2 comprised 5 bilingual health care practitioners from a variety of disciplines, including otolaryngology, audiology, psychology and aviation medicine, who regularly treated patients with vertigo. all except the psychologist had received specialised training in vestibular disorders and were familiar with the vss. phase 1 materials and procedures – delphi consensus rounds 1 and 2 panel 1 answered a list of questions on the language, grammar and vocabulary of items in the translated vss. as the scale was to be directed at patients, the translation needed to be comprehensible to lay individuals. questions for panel 2 centred on the applicability of items to the afrikaans patient population to which practitioners were exposed, as well as to verify the use of vocabulary chosen to explain terms such as ‘giddy’. panel 2 was also polled with regard to words commonly used by their patients to describe the experience of vertigo. both panels were consulted regarding the afrikaans translation and equivalence with the english original. panelists selected answers from a 5-point likert scale ranging from ‘strongly agree’ to ‘strongly disagree’; in addition qualitative comments were invited. examples of questions asked of the panels are indicated in box 1 below. • a patient who reads ‘swewing’ will make the association with a feeling of ‘swimming, floating or soaring’ (item 7). • do you think that ‘dofheid’ encompasses the essence of visual ‘blurring’ (item 13)? box 1. example of questions posed to the panellists participating in the delphi consensus procedure, round 1. there is a lack of agreement in the literature as to what percentage is acceptable as consensus, with values ranging from 55% to 100% (powell, 2002). an 80% majority was chosen as it represented a robust majority leaving less room for errors (hardy et al., 2004). when 80% consensus was achieved no changes were made to the initial translation. round 2 of the delphi consensus addressed items from round 1 upon which consensus had not been achieved. panel members were able to re-evaluate their opinion based on the responses and suggestions from both panels that were presented verbatim (greatorex & dexter, 2000). respondents had to ‘agree’ or ‘disagree’ with each item. for round 2 a majority consensus of ≥51% finalised the changes. an example of one of the questions from round 2 appears in box 2. the results of the delphi consensus are the avss (appendix 1), and a list of afrikaans words used to describe the subjective experience of vertigo (see box 3 in the ‘results’ section). for item 4, both panel 1 and panel 2 agreed by majority that ‘ neerval ’ is an appropriate translation for ‘fall’. however, other suggestions were made. choose the item that you most agree with. do you agree with a) ‘ neerval ’ agree/ disagree or the suggestion b) ‘ omval ’ agree/ disagree box 2. example of questions posed to the panellists participating in the delphi consensus procedure, round 2. phase 2: pilot study of the avss phase 2 piloted the avss and used descriptive correlational statistics to analyse the results. there were two aims for phase 2: first, to assess and describe the relationship between the participants’ presenting symptoms and their scores on the avss for the anxiety and vertigo subscales; and second, to demonstrate whether the avss could discriminate between vertiginious and control participants. phase 2 participants as both a non-vertiginous group and group of participants with vertigo were sought, all adult patients attending ent outpatient services were asked if they were interested in enrolling in the research study. phase 2 participants who reported that their first language was afrikaans and who were capable of completing the avss unassisted were selected using a purposive non-randomised sampling method. the delphi consensus procedure generated key terms used to describe vertigo (see box 3), and symptoms reported by participants within the last 3 months had to match one or more of these terms for participant inclusion in the vertiginous group; control subjects reported no vertigo within the same period. eighty-five patients gave consent; subsequently 13 were excluded because the questionnaire was returned incomplete. the sample consisted of 72 participants, of whom 50 were female. vertigo was present in 41 participants and 31 were controls. the age of participants with vertigo ranged from 20 to 82 years (mean 49 years), and that of the controls from 27 to 81 years (mean 45 years). the median schooling level was grade 10, mode of grade 12 (n=72). ethical clearance the research protocol was designed in accordance with the declaration of helsinki (world medical association, 2008). ethical clearance was obtained from the institution’s human research ethics committee as well as from the two hospitals at which data were collected. informed consent was obtained from all participants for both phases of the study. professor lucy yardley granted permission for the use of the vss, its translation and naming as the afrikaans vertigo symptom scale (yardley, personal communication, january 2009). phase 2 materials and procedures once informed consent was obtained, participants completed the screening questionnaire assessing the main reason for the visit: presence of previous and current symptoms of vertigo, panic, anxiety, depression and medication use. the avss was completed and participants were divided into vertiginous and control groups based on the results of the screening questionnaire. statistical analysis the diagnosis was recorded from the hospital folder. data were initially entered into microsoft office excel 2007 (v. 12.0.6300.5000). data from 10% of the sample were re-analysed at random to check for reliability of data capturing. the statistica (v. 8) package and statistical package for social sciences (spss) (v. 15) were used for statistical analysis. cronbach’s alpha assessed internal consistency. the mcnemar test determined the classification congruence between the participants’ presenting complaint of vertigo, or the lack thereof, and their diagnosis. the adjusted t-test and mann-whitney u were conducted. the receiver-operating characteristic (roc) curve was used as a visual index of the accuracy of the avss and analysed sensitivity and specificity. results phase 1 the researchers who performed the initial translations found that the two independent preliminary translations were very similar. furthermore, no substantial differences were found when the initial forward and back translations were compared. in round 1, the first-language afrikaans panel reached consensus on 22 of 31 items. the health care professional panel agreed on 22 of 32 questions. round 2 consisted of 21 questions, 19 of which had suggestions included. consensus was achieved for all of the 21 questions in round 2 and the avss was finalised. participants mentioned that the layout of the avss made it challenging to complete, so the format (but not the content) was reworked to make it more accessible. for example, response options were arranged in boxes for participants to tick or circle. the terms suggested by the panellists for afrikaans synonyms for vertigo appear in box 3. • rondomtalie, tuimel, bollemakiesie, mallemeule, tol • ‘draai’ (patient or the environment) • draaiduiseligheid • dronk/kop-dronkheid • ‘ek beweeg hierdie kant toe, die wêreld anderkant toe.’ box 3. words and terms that were obtained from qualitative feedback in round 1 of the delphi technique describing the symptom of vertigo. phase 2 the vss evaluates two areas – the experience of vertigo, dizziness and imbalance (ver subscale) and symptoms of anxiety and related psychological problems, the anxiety and autonomic symptom scale (aa subscale). cronbach’s alpha indicated good internal consistency on the ver subscale (α=0.8822) and the aa subscale (α=0.9248), i.e. the results obtained on the two subscales indicated that the scale will elicit consistent results, rather than results obtained by subject or item variance. the mcnemar test was used to analyse the classification congruence between complaints of vertigo and the expectation of the symptom based on the diagnosis recorded in the hospital notes. four of the 72 participants included in the final analysis (those with complete avss scores) were incorrectly categorised as having vertigo when in fact they did not. the mcnemar test (0.98; p=0.3211) found no significant difference between the participants’ complaint of vertigo and the expectation of the symptom based on diagnosis. this minimal difference in classification indicated good categorisation of vertiginous participants and controls, based on participants’ presenting complaints. for the vertiginous participants the score of symptoms of dizziness, vertigo or imbalance was relatively high, with the ver subscale showing mean 19.902, standard deviation (sd) 12.047, n=41, where the highest possible score obtainable on the ver is 76. most of the vertiginous sample (54%) scored on all three features of dizziness: a feeling of spinning or moving around, being light-headed, swimmy or giddy, and a feeling of unsteadiness (items 1, 7 and 18 on the avss). in contrast, no participant from the control group reported all three classic features of dizziness with increased occurrence (scores of 3 or 4 on the avss). the control sample yielded a significantly lower mean score overall, with the ver subscale showing mean 3.742, sd 3.838, n=31. this would suggest that the ver is able to differentiate reliably between those with and without vertigo. figure 2 shows histograms of the ver data for both samples. in addition to the presence of symptoms of dizziness, over one-third of the vertiginous sample complained of symptoms of autonomic nervous system arousal and/or anxiety; checking items such as the presence of hot or cold spells, heart palpitations, and presyncope (items 3, 10 and 22). a relatively high mean score overall for the aa subscale was obtained (mean 26.829, sd 16.269, n=41), where the highest possible score on this subscale is 60. the control group reflected a lower overall mean score, with the following obtained: mean 13.065, sd 9.284, n=31. interestingly, none of the participants in the control group reported presyncope. figure 3 shows histograms of the scores obtained on the aa for both samples. successful differentiation between the participant groups was therefore obtained on both subscales of the avss. further statistical analysis was conducted, and both the parametric and non-parametric measures used indicated significant statistical differences on both subscales between the samples with t-test and mann-whitney u values less than p=0.05. between both samples, the following results were obtained on the adjusted t-test and mann-whitney u non-parametric analysis: ver [t (50.25)=-8.065, p<0.001; mann-whitney u (49)=0.000, p<0.001] and aa: [t (65.64)=4.529, p<0.001; mann-whitney u (316.5)=0.000, p<0.001]. the p-scores obtained for the t-test and mann-whitney u are substantially less than 0.001, which is less than the set criteria for statistical significance, increasing the significance of the results obtained. an roc curve (figure 4) was constructed in an attempt to establish cut-off scores that would distinguish between cases of vertigo and non-cases. the avss shows good sensitivity and specificity for both subscales and as a whole. the ver has better sensitivity in identifying vertiginous versus control participants than the aa’s ability to identify those with anxiety from those without. the ver has an optimal cut-off suggested at 7.5, which yields a sensitivity of 0.902 and specificity of 0.097. the aa did not indicate an optimal cut-off and 17.5 was suggested, as the best balance for sensitivity is 0.683 and specificity is 0.258 at this level. to conclude, the entire avss presents with relatively good sensitivity and specificity (demonstrated by the second line from the reference line on the roc). discussion there are various methods of evaluating the translation of an instrument, ranging from simple forwardand back-translations to subjecting the translation to a process of review using a technique such as the delphi. in this study a delphi consensus procedure, using panels of first-language afrikaans speakers and bilingual health care professionals, yielded the avss which was piloted with vertiginous and control participants. although the focus of the discussion is on the results of the pilot study, the delphi is discussed briefly. delphi consensus procedures have a number of features that may either enrich or weaken a study. they are cost-effective, and panel members are usually selected for their strengths and are not unduly influenced by each other. however, a lack of standardisation in the method, including decisions about when consensus has been reached, lack of test-retest reliability and possible selection bias (hasson et al., 2000), all warrant concern. although there is no agreement on what to accept as consensus in the literature (hasson et al., 2000), a conservative figure was chosen for consensus in the first round, favouring caution. in the second round, consensus was achieved for all items. there were limitations in terms of the small number of participants on each panel. it is possible that the panels’ opinions were not representative of a wider first-language or expert population, raising the possibility of selection bias. in addition, the use of a first-language lay panel could be queried with regard to their value; however, it was felt that the final translation should be accessible in terms of vocabulary to a projected patient population, and that the lay panel would bring a different perspective to that of the health care professionals. furthermore, no formal tests to establish language competence of any of the participants were conducted. although the limitations of the delphi consensus procedure require acknowledgement, they are somewhat ameliorated by its use only to confirm a translation of an already validated questionnaire. the researchers believe that concurrent validity has been established as a result of the avss’s ability to discriminate between individuals presenting with and without vertigo. furthermore, cronbach’s alpha suggested internal consistency within the scale. clinical utility of the avss because of the array of symptoms and aetiologies with which a vertiginous patient may present, the underpinning of a competent assessment is a thorough case history (mégnigbêto et al., 2001; yardley et al., 1992). however, in south africa clinicians may encounter challenges in obtaining a case history as a result of linguistic issues. a questionnaire such as the avss explores symptoms of dizziness and vertigo, associated autonomic nervous system symptoms and anxiety-related factors. the avss has proven reliable and could be useful for clinicians to categorise patients’ subtypes of dizziness. it bears reiteration that precise symptom definition, plus identification of associated symptoms such as anxiety, is essential for effective diagnosis. further, self-assessment scales require patients to be reflective about their complaint. as the avss covers a wide range of symptoms, the patient is asked to consider several aspects of his/her condition. the avss may create an opportunity for the clinician to explore specific areas, which may assist in keeping the consultation focused and time-efficient. for example, identification of anxiety is essential for successful management (luxon, 2004), and high scores on the aa subscale would alert the clinician to explore this during the clinical encounter and refer the patient if necessary. even when physical symptoms cannot be controlled optimally, such as in the case of ménière’s disease, recognition and management of psychological distress can result in improvements in the quality of life (kirby & yardley, 2008). significant statistical differences were found between the samples for both subscales embedded in the avss. the roc curve suggested that the avss is sensitive and specific in correctly identifying true cases of participants complaining of vertigo. however, a clinical tool may have considerable power in identifying those patients whom it aims to identify, yet be of little value when it comes to patient care (zwieg & campbell, 1993). as one of the primary concerns in management of vertigo is delineating its true nature, which in turn will dictate subsequent treatment and referral options, instruments with a high hit rate are desirable. the avss could be used at various levels of care and ensure that patients are referred to the appropriate health care professional – in this case vertiginous patients to audiologists and otologists. in spite of the avss being statistically robust, it is apparent that not all tools are suitable for all patients. in this study, 13 participants were excluded because the avss had not been completed in full. qualitative feedback from these individuals suggested that some had difficulty with the format, which followed the published english version. this resulted in a new layout for the scale, with boxes for participants to enter their responses, which had a clearer appearance. however, when the challenges of a self-assessment scale in cases where literacy may be an issue are considered, the avss in its present format may still be too daunting for some patients. for individuals with low general literacy levels faced with material presented in a different language, written documents used in health care may give rise to frustration. interestingly, clinicians are thought to overestimate their patients’ literacy skills, leading to more difficulties (schmidt von wühlisch & pascoe, 2010). it is possible that the avss may not be of practical use in some clinical settings, depending on the functional and health literacy of the patient population. the study did not explore the threat to validity should the scale be administered by a health care professional rather than self-administered, but this could be investigated at a later stage. the generalisability of the study is limited by the size and centralised nature of the sample. study data cannot account for the range of dizzy patients who could potentially consult a clinician. the state hospitals were both located in an urban centre and it is possible that rural participants may have yielded substantially different results. in addition participants did not undergo objective testing, thus limiting the correlation between the participants’ diagnosis and their presenting complaint of vertigo, or lack thereof. however, a counter-argument to this is that objective testing often does not prove a diagnosis or even the presence of a patient’s symptoms (kerr, 2005; mendel, bergenius & langius, 1999). it is possible that a hawthorne effect exists. participants may have reported more symptoms, or more severe symptoms, on the avss in the hope that they might receive preferential treatment or have their medical needs given more priority. however, it was pointed out to each potential participant that the researchers would neither assess nor manage patients’ conditions and that this would be attended to by the regular staff. as this was a pilot study, future research should include a larger sample of participants, from a variety of settings. furthermore, as there is no other suitable afrikaans questionnaire with which to compare the avss, construct validity was not evaluated. it is therefore recommended that another scale such as the dizziness handicap inventory (dhi) (jacobson & newman, 1990) be translated and piloted. the results of the two scales could be compared for information regarding the validity of the avss. furthermore, the results of the self-assessment scales could be correlated with findings on clinical or objective examination. in south africa, a range of clinicians may be involved in the management of patients with vestibular involvement, including otolaryngologists, neurologists, audiologists with vestibular training, physiotherapists and psychologists. all of these professionals will approach the vertiginous patient from a different perspective relative to their training. the study revealed that there does not appear to be an afrikaans word that appropriately implies the symptom of vertigo, so ‘draaiduiseligheid’ is suggested as suitable for use within clinical settings. the term adequately conveys a sense of dizziness or of being light-headed, while incorporating the vital aspect of a hallucination of movement. consistent use of terminology within and between disciplines, as well as use of the avss, may offer something to bridge professional differences and aid the clinician who is not specialised in the area of vestibular disorders. clinical reasoning regarding both the definition of the presenting symptoms and the results from the subscales of the avss may in turn make referrals more rational and appropriate. conclusion the avss is able to detect classic symptoms of vertiginous distress, which often include associated symptoms of autonomic nervous system arousal. the roc measure indicated that the avss presents with good sensitivity and specificity, and therefore demonstrates good discriminative ability in identifying patients with vertigo. hence it is an ideal first option in patient self-assessment and can appropriately confirm the presence of symptoms, explore facets of anxiety and direct appropriate management and future referrals. the avss will complement the case history, which in turn will support more accurate diagnosis. it should be noted that the english and translated vss were designed as self-assessment scales, and this could render them inaccessible for a sizeable proportion of the population who do not have functional literacy. the word ‘draaiduiseligheid’ is a useful addition to the clinical armamentarium as it captures a description of movement and disorientation to the environment. as symptom definition is a fundamental first step in discriminating between dizziness and vertigo, and thus directing assessment and management decisions, it is hoped that this will prove to be a useful contribution. considering the wide use of the vss around the world in specialist clinics, the avss has the potential to enhance the assessment of vertigo and attendant vertigo-related anxiety in the afrikaans-speaking population. acknowledgements. the researchers would like to extend their gratitude to professor lucy yardley for permission to use and translate the vss, and ms anneli hardy for her statistical analysis. we also wish to acknowledge the contribution of the panellists and the participants at the two institutions at which the study was conducted, and the two anonymous reviewers for their constructive comments. references aslan, s., ceylan, a., kemaloglu, y.k., & goksu, n. (2003). validity and reliability of the turkish version of the vertigo handicap questionnaire (vhq) in a group of patients with vertigo. gazi medical journal, 14, 167-173. bennett, m. (2008). the vertigo case history. in g.p. jacobson & n.t. shepard (eds.). balance function assessment and management (pp. 45-62). san diego: plural publishing. best, c., eckhardt-henn, a., diener, g., bense, s., breuer, p., & dieterich, m. (2006). interaction of somatoform and vestibular disorders. journal of neurology, neurosurgery & psychiatry, 77, 658-664. cohen, h.s., & kimball, k.t. (2003). increased independence and decreased vertigo after vestibular rehabilitation. otolaryngology head and neck surgery, 128, 60-70. danermark, b., cieza, a., gange, j-p., gimigliano, f., granberg, s., hickson, l., et al. (2010). international classification of functioning, disability and health core sets for hearing loss: a discussion paper and invitation. international journal of audiology, 49, 256-262. de villiers, m.r., de villiers, p.j.t., & kent, a.p. (2005). the delphi technique in health science education research. medical teacher, 27, 639-643. faag, c., bergenius, j., forsberg, c., & langius-eklöf, a. (2007). symptoms experienced by patients with peripheral vestibular disorders: evaluation of the vertigo symptom scale for clinical application. clinical otolaryngology & allied sciences, 32, 440-446. greatorex, j., & dexter, t. (2000). an accessible analytical approach for investigating what happens between the rounds of a delphi study. journal of advanced nursing, 32, 1016-1024. hardy, d.j., o’brien, a.p., gaskin, c.j., o’brien, a.j., morrison-ngatai, e., skews, g., et al. (2004). practical implication of the delphi technique in a bicultural mental health nursing study in new zealand. journal of advanced nursing, 46, 95-109. harpham, t., reichenheim, m., oser, r., thomas, e., hamid, n., jaswal, s., et al. (2003). how to do (or not to do). measuring mental health in a cost-effective manner. health policy and planning, 18, 344-349. hasson, f., keeney, s., & mckenna, h. (2000). research guidelines for delphi survey technique. journal of advanced nursing, 32, 1008-1015. jacobson, g.p., & newman, c.w. (1990). the development of the dizziness handicap inventory. archives of otolaryngology – head & neck surgery, 116, 424-427. jelsma, j., & ferguson, g. (2004). the determinants of self-reported health-related quality of life in a culturally and socially diverse south african community. bulletin of the world health organisation, 82, 206-212. kerr, a.g. (2005). assessment of vertigo. annals of the academy of medicine of singapore, 34, 285-288. kirby, s.e., & yardley, l. (2008). understanding psychological distress in ménière’s disease: a systematic review. psychology, health & medicine, 13, 257-273. luxon, l.m. (2004). evaluation and management of the dizzy patient. journal of neurology, neurosurgery & neuropsychiatry, 75, 45-52. mcpherson, d.l., & whitaker, s.r. (2001). dxx: disequilibrium and aging. in j.a. goebel (ed.). practical management of the dizzy patient (pp. 269-297). philadelphia: lippincott williams & wilkins. mégnigbêto, c.a., sauvage, j., & launois, r. (2001). validation clinique d’une échelle du vertige: european evaluation of vertigo scale (eev): a clinical validation study. revue laryngology otology & rhinology, 122, 95-102. meli, a., zimatore, g., badaracco, c., de angelis, e., & tufarelli, d. (2007). effects of vestibular rehabilitation therapy on emotional aspects in chronic vestibular patients. journal of psychosomatic research, 63, 185-190. mendel, b., bergenius, j., & langius, a. (1999). dizziness symptom severity and impact on daily living as perceived by patients suffering from peripheral vestibular disorder. clinical otolaryngology & allied sciences, 24, 286-293. panday, s., kathard, h., pillay, m., & govender, c. (2007). development of a zulu speech reception threshold test for zulu first language speakers in kwazulu natal. south african journal of communication disorders, 54, 111-122. powell, c. (2002). the delphi technique: myths and realities. methodological issues in nursing research, 41, 376-382. schmidt von wühlisch, f., & pascoe, m. (2010). maximising health literacy and client recall of clinical information: an exploratory study of clients and speech language pathologists. south african journal of communication disorders, 57, 22-32. shepard, n.t. (2007). dizziness and balance disorders: the role of the history and laboratory studies in diagnosis and management. the asha leader. retrieved on 23 august 2011 from http://www.asha.org/publications/leader/2007/070529/f070529a.html south africa info. afrikaans. retrieved on 23 august 2011 from http://www.southafrica.info/about/people/language/html statistics south africa (2004). provincial profile 2004, western cape. retrieved on 4 january 2009 from http://www.statssa.gov.za tschan, r., wiltink, j., best, c., bense, s., dieterich, m., beutel, m.e., et al. (2008). validation of the german version of the vertigo symptom scale (vss) in patients with organic or somatoform dizziness and healthy controls. journal of neurology, 255, 1168-1175. webster, k., cella, d., & yost, k. (2003). the functional assessment of chronic illness therapy (facit) measurement system; properties, applications and interpretation. health and quality of life outcomes. retrieved on 23 august 2011 from www.hqlo.com/content/pdf/1477-7525-1-79.pdf wiltink, j., tschan, r., michal, m., subic-wrana, c., eckhardt-henn, a., dieterich, m., et al. (2009). dizziness: anxiety, health care utilisation and health behaviour – results from a representative german community survey. journal of psychosomatic research, 66, 417-424. world medical association (2008). declaration of helsinki. retrieved on 17 may 2010 from www.wma.net/en/30publications/10policies/p3/17c.pdf yardley, l., luxon, l.m., & haacke, n.p. (1994). a longitudinal study of symptoms, anxiety and subjective well-being in patients with vertigo. clinical otolaryngology & allied sciences, 19, 109-116. yardley, l., masson, e., verschuur, c., haacke, n., & luxon, l. (1992). symptoms, anxiety and handicap in dizzy patients: development of the vertigo symptom scale. journal of psychosomatic research, 36, 731-741. zwieg, m.h., & campbell, g. (1993). receiver-operating characteristic (roc) plots: a fundamental evaluation tool in clinical medicine. clinical chemistry, 39, 561-577. fig. 1. flow chart depicting the translation of the vertigo symptom scale into afrikaans. fig. 2. histograms depicting the spread of scores obtained on the ver subscale. the control results are in the left diagram. scores on the ver are found on the x-axis with the number of participants found on the y-axis. note that the control sample’s scores are substantially lower than those obtained by the vertiginous sample. fig. 3. histograms depicting the spread of scores obtained on the aa subscale. scores for the control group are displayed in the left diagram. scores obtained on the aa are found on the x-axis. overall, the vertiginous sample scored higher on the aa. fig. 4. the roc curve obtained for the avss, including both the ver and aa subscales. appendix 1. afrikaanse vertigo simptome skaal instruksies: omkring asseblief die gepaste nommer om aan te toon ongeveer hoeveel keer jy die volgende simptome, op die lys, ervaar het gedurende die laaste 12 maande (of sedert die duiseligheid begin het, indien jou duiseligheid minder as ’n jaar gelede begin het). die verskeidenheid van keuses is: 0 1 2 3 4 nooit enkele kere (1 3 maal ’n jaar) verskeie kere (4 12 maal ’n jaar) redelik gereeld (gemiddeld, meer as 1 maal per maand) baie gereeld (gemiddeld, meer as 1 maal per week) hoe gereeld gedurende die afgelope 12 maande het jy die volgende simptome gehad: nooit enkele kere (1 3 maal ń jaar verskeie kere (4 12 maal ń jaar redelik gereeld (gemiddeld, meer as 1 maal per maand) baie gereeld (gemiddeld, meer as 1 maal per week) 1. ’n gevoel dat alles draai of in die rondte beweeg, vir ’n tydperk van: [beantwoord asseblief a) tot e)] a) minder as 2 minute b) tot en met 20 minute c) 20 minute tot 1 uur d) ’n aantal ure e) meer as 12 ure 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 2. pyn in die hart of bors area 0 1 2 3 4 3. warm of koue gloede 0 1 2 3 4 4. onvas op jou voete, so erg dat jy omval 0 1 2 3 4 5. naarheid (siek voel), ’n draai gevoel in die maag 0 1 2 3 4 6. spanning/seerheid in jou spiere 0 1 2 3 4 7. ’n gevoel van lighoofdigheid, ’n gevoel van ‘swewing’ of duiseligheid, vir ’n tydperk van: [beantwoord asseblief a) tot e)] a) minder as 2 minute b) tot en met 20 minute c) 20 minute tot 1 uur d) ’n aantal ure e) meer as 12 ure 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 8. bewerigheid, rillings 0 1 2 3 4 9. ’n gevoel van drukking in die oor/ore 0 1 2 3 4 10. hartkloppings of -versnellings 0 1 2 3 4 11. braking 0 1 2 3 4 12. ’n swaar gevoel in die arms of bene 0 1 2 3 4 13. visuele versteurings (bv. dofheid, flikkering, kolle voor die oë) 0 1 2 3 4 14. hoofpyn of ’n gevoel van drukking in die kop 0 1 2 3 4 15. onvermoeë om behoorlik, sonder ondersteuning, te staan of te stap 0 1 2 3 4 16. moeilike asemhaling, kortasem 0 1 2 3 4 17. verlies van konsentrasie of geheue 0 1 2 3 4 18. onvas op jou voete, besig om balans te verloor, vir ’n tydperk van: [beantwoord asseblief a) tot e)] a) minder as 2 minute b) tot en met 20 minute c) 20 minute tot 1 uur d) ’n aantal ure e) meer as 12 ure 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 0 1 2 3 4 19. tinteling, prikkeling of lamheid in dele van die liggaam 0 1 2 3 4 20. pyne in jou laerug area 0 1 2 3 4 21. oormatige sweet 0 1 2 3 4 22. voel flou, besig om bewussyn te verloor 0 1 2 3 4 13 attitudes of a group of south african speech-language pathologists towards stutterers and stuttering therapy belinda baker, eleanor ross and joan girson department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract the present study aimed to examine the attitudes of a group of south african speech-language pathologists towards stuttering and stuttering therapy. further aims were to investigate whether a stereotype of stutterers was found among these speech-language pathologists, and to determine whether there was any relationship between the attitudes held about stutterers, and the therapists' training and experience. a random probability sample of respondents was selected from the population of speech therapists registered with the interim medical and dental council of south africa (i.m.d.c.s.a.). a self-administered mailed questionnaire was employed to realise the aims of the study. the main result of this investigation indicated that almost 50% of the sample of qualified clinicians surveyed, viewed stutterers as a group characterised by specific personality traits and psychological problems. this belief held true irrespective of the number of years of experience working in the field, the time of graduation, the frequency of treating stutterers, or the training emphasis. implications of these results are considered with respect to student training, continuing education of qualified practitioners and future research. opsomming die huidige navorsing is gemik om die houding van 'n groep suid-afrikaanse spraak-taalpatoloe te ondersoek. verdere doelstellings was om ondersoek in te stel na 'n moontlike verwantskap tussen die opleiding en ondervinding van terapeute en hulle houding teenoor hakkelaars, asook of hulle hakkelaars stereotipeer. 'n waarskynlikheids-toevalssteekproef van respondente is uit die suid-afrikaanse tussentydse mediese en tandheelkundige raad (s.a.t.m.t.r.) se geregistreerde terapeute geselekteer. 'n selfopgestelde posvraelys is gebruik vir die doeleindes van hierdie studie. die hoofresultaat het aangedui dat amper 50% van respondente hakkelaars as 'n groep met spesifieke persoonlikheidseienskappe, asook sielkundige probleme beskou, ongeag van die aantal jare van werksondervinding, die datum van graduering, die aantal hakkelaars behandel en die opleidingsklem. die implikasies vir studente-opleiding, voortgesette onderrig aan gekwalifiseerde terapeute en toekomstige navorsing word bespreek. keywords: attitudes, speech-language pathologists, stutterers, stuttering therapy the relationship between attitudes and stuttering has long been highlighted in the literature (peters & guitar 1991). in fact, various authorities have found that the attitudes of speech therapists towards their stuttering patients is a crucial factor in therapy (cooper & cooper, 1985b; watson, 1995). these findings highlight the need for speech therapists to approach stuttering therapy with a positive attitude (daly, simon & burnett-stolnack, 1995). in contrast with this viewpoint, other research has suggested the presence of a negative stereotype of stutterers, lb stereotype a group is to think and refer to all members of that group as though they were the same (jones, 1977). according to ham (1990a), it is the perception of the general public that stutterers are less adequate people than non-stutterers, and non-stutterers often describe stutterers with mainly negative characteristics (kalinowski, lerman & watt, 1987). negative stereotypes may serve as self-fulfilling prophecies, influencing the stutterers' selfimage and behaviour (turnbaugh, guitar & hoffman, 1979). the expectations that people have, influence the behaviour of the person holding the expectation and the person about whom the expectation is held, as in most instances the latter party is aware of the stereotype (jones, 1977). it is not only the general public which has negative views of stutterers. speech therapy students have been found to be prejudiced against stutterers (barbosa, schiefer & chiari, 1995). several studies have found that speech therapists use predominantly negative personality traits to describe stutterers (yairi & williams, 1970; woods & williams, 1971). negative stereotypes of stutterers as people with psychological problems appeared to persist irrespective of the severity of the disorder (turnbaugh et al., 1979); the age of the patient (woods & williams, 1971) or the clinicians' degree of exposure to stuttering clients (woods & williams, 1976). in 1975 cooper devised the clinicians' attitudes towards stuttering (cats) inventory as he felt that existing attitude scales did not adequately assess clinicians' attitudes (cooper, 1996). in 1983 and 1991 cooper and cooper administered the cats inventory to a sample of american clinicians and found that 50% and 58% respecdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 tively of their respondents believed that stutterers possess characteristic personality traits (cooper & cooper, 1985b & cooper 1996). the negative attitude held by clinicians could impede the stutterer in therapy (cooper & cooper, 1985a), and hinder the therapeutic relationship (starkweather, 1982). clinicians' negative attitudes may also add to the stutterers' need to hide their speech disorder, and may impede any therapy which requires patients to advertise their stutter (woods & williams, 1976). in addition, the therapist's reaction may add to the client's sense of guilt associated with belonging to such a negatively perceived group (leahy, 1994). young stutterers may also learn a negative stereotype of stutterers from the important adults in their lives (rustin & cook, 1995), including the therapist. stereotypes of stutterers contradict research which has repeatedly shown that stutterers are not a homogeneous population and that a "stuttering personality" does not exist (andrews et al., 1983; lass et al., 1992). stutterers themselves have opposed the idea that they are a homogeneous group which can be stereotyped (fransella, 1968 as cited by ham, 1990a) and there has been no significant difference in the self-descriptions of stutterers and nonstutterers (kalinowski et al., 1987). in view of the reported negative attitudes of speech therapists towards stutterers, it is not surprising that stuttering has been described as one of the least favoured speech disorders to treat (st louis & durrenberger 1993). this finding may reflect the fact that therapists often report feeling inadequately prepared for treating stutterers (st louis & durrenberger, 1993); and incompetent in managing stutterers (mallard, gardner & downey, 1988; cooper & cooper, 1985b). therapists also mention feeling pessimistic about the outcome of stuttering therapy (andrews et al., 1983). andrews et al. (1983) claim that the persistence of a negative stereotype of stutterers held by qualified clinicians may reflect the lack of assimilation of new knowledge about stuttering in training procedures, coupled with the continued use of approaches that are more than 30 years old. they state that in the past 10 20 years studies have shown that stutterers as people are no different to anyone else and that stuttering therapy can be highly effective (andrews et al., 1983). training may be a significant contributing factor in the development of attitudes towards stutterers, as attitudes are learnt (triandis, 1971). contradictory findings have been documented in the literature regarding the effects of practical experience and training on students' attitudes towards stuttering. on the one hand, st. louis and durrenberger (1993) found that increased practical experience in stuttering therapy may result in clinicians adopting a more positive attitude. on the other hand, increased academic training may lead to students becoming more pessimistic about their competence (st louis & lass, 1981). st louis and lass (1981) concluded that the fear of stuttering is embedded in training programmes. there may be a fundamental problem with the basic preparation students receive. hence, a change in the type or direction of programmes may be required if additional academic courses are unsuccessful in enhancing clinicians' confidence. the nature of peoples' attitudes towards stuttering should be amenable to change if they are exposed to the belinda baker, eleanor ross & joan girson appropriate knowledge and experience regarding stuttering (emerick, 1960). unfortunately, leith (1971) found that students often have limited practical experiences with stutterers in their training. using current knowledge about the existing stereotype of stutterers, leahy (1994) was able to modify student clinicians' attitudes as a direct result of her training procedures. she found that experience in treating stutterers at an undergraduate level, as well as group therapy experiences, were significant contributing factors in changing negative stereotypes of stutterers. st louis and lass (1981: 68) recommended that "one potentially fruitful avenue of future research would be to determine the extent to which various types of information or clinical experience influence attitudes towards stuttering. .. .such information would assist training programs and instructors in designing programs and courses better able to shape student attitudes in healthy, productive ways". this knowledge would appear to be especially relevant to the south african context where there is a limited supply of speech therapy services and resources available to the stuttering population. in 1987 van der merwe used a survey questionnaire to investigate the attitudes of south african speech therapists towards stuttering, stutterers and stuttering therapy. she found that afrikaans clinicians, as well as clinicians with more practical experience in treating stutterers, tended to have a more positive approach to therapy and were optimistic about the efficacy of therapy. the present study aimed to look more closely at the possible relationship between training and experience, and the personality attributes that clinicians believe stutterers to possess. methodology aims 1. to examine the attitudes of a group of south african speech-language pathologists towards stutterers and stuttering therapy. 2. to investigate whether a stereotype of stutterers was held by these speech-language pathologists. 3. to determine whether there was any relationship between the attitudes clinicians held about the personalities of stutterers and these clinicians' training and experience. research design in order to investigate the aims of the study a cross sectional survey research design was adopted which utilised a mailed questionnaire. subjects subject selection criteria three hundred names were randomly selected from the 1024 speech-language pathologists registered -with the interim medical and dental council of south africa as at june 1995. a total of 123 subjects returned useable questionnaires, representing a response rate of 41%. according to de vaus (1991), for purposes of analysis, a 10% response represents an adequate proportion of the total population. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) attitudes of a group of south african speech-language pathologists towards stutterers and stuttering therapy description of questionnaire 1 a five-page questionnaire was designed by the researcher, based on questionnaires devised by cooper (1996), van der merwe (1987) and mallard, gardner and downey (1988). the questionnaire comprised the following sections:1. the "clinicians' attitudes towards stuttering (cats) inventory" 2. biographical information 3. experience with stuttering therapy. each of these sections is described separately as follows: clinicians'attitudes towards stuttering (cats) inventory the cats inventory consists of 50 attitudinal statements regarding eight areas of stuttering. respondents indicate their reactions to the statements via a five point strength of agreement scale ranging from "strongly agree" through "undecided" to "strongly disagree" (cooper & cooper, 1996). in the analysis these responses were collapsed to form a three point strength of agreement scale, as the differences between the "strongly agree or disagree" and "agree or disagree" categories are highly subjective. van der merwe's modified version of the cats inventory and her afrikaans translation were used by the researcher where appropriate. based on pre-test procedures further modifications were made. the questionnaire aimed to determine the attitudes of south african speech-language therapists towards: a) the etiology of stuttering. b) early intervention. c) efficacy of stuttering therapy. d) the personality of stutterers. e) clinicians who treat stutterers. f) teachers' and others' reactions to stuttering. g)/various therapy techniques. h) parents of stutterers. biographical information a biographical section devised by the researcher was used to obtain background!information on the subjects regarding their age, sex, year of qualification, training, years of professional experience and where that experience was received. this section was based largely on areas targeted by mallard, gardner and downey (1988) and van der merwe (1987). experience with stuttering therapy this section consisted of eight questions probing undergraduate and professional experience with stutterers. two open-ended questions were included to obtain qualitatively rich information. data analysis descriptive statistics were used as there were insufficient data in each cell of the cross tabulation table to warrant the use of inferential statistical procedures such as 15 chi square. frequency distributions were utilised to determine the percentage of speech-language pathologists who fell within each of the strength of agreement categories for each statement. the biographical information was summarized using these statistics. content analysis was utilised to analyse open-ended questions. the possibility of a relationship between clinicians' attitudes towards stutterers as people and the clinicians' training and professional experience was investigated by means of cross tabulation. results and discussion attitudes towards stutterers and stuttering therapy "etiology" the overwhelming majority of respondents, i.e., 86.2% disagreed with the statement that stuttering is a relatively simple disorder of dyssynchrony of the speech musculature, table 1 biographical profile of respondents (n = 123) no. % gender male 1 0.8 female 122 99.2 age under 26 yrs 30 24.4 2 6 3 5 59 48.0 3 6 4 5 25 20.3 4 6 5 5 6 04.9 55 + 3 02.4 date of 1990 1995 52 42.3 graduation 1986 1990 17 13.8 1981 1985 24 19.6 1976 1980 11 08.95 1971 1975 11 08.95 1966 1970 2 01.6 1961 1965 3 02.4 1955 1960 3 02.4 years of 1 5 50 40.7 experience 6 10 26 21.1 11 15 24 19.6 1 6 2 0 12 09.8 21 25 5 04.0 2 6 3 0 4 03.2 31 35 1 00.8 36 + 1 00.8 language of english 75 61.0 instruction afrikaans 48 39,0 majority of hospital 30 14.4 professional school 51 24.4 experience special school 37 17.7 (n = 209) * private practice 71 34.0 university 12 05.7 other 8 03.8 * numbers do not total 123 as respondents had worked in more than one setting. percentages are given in terms of the percentage of responses in this category. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 belinda baker, eleanor ross & joan girson while 89.4% agreed that stuttering results from multiple co-existing factors. as many as 83% disagreed with the view that parents are the primary cause of stuttering in their children, suggesting a decline in support for johnson's diagnosogenic model of etiology which states that a child's normal speech pattern becomes abnormal through the anxious reaction of parents (peters & guitar, 1991). although there is a dearth of literature to support johnson's theory (andrews et al., 1983), 8.9% of the south african sample of clinicians still held parents primarily responsible for stuttering in their children. this belief is maintained despite the literature over the past 20 years which suggests that the parent-child relationship of a stutterer is no different from that of a non-stutterer (andrews et al, 1983). in fact, onslow (1996) advocates counselling parents and providing them with literature on the physiological nature of stuttering, in an attempt to alleviate the guilt which has plagued the parents of stutterers for years. despite the small proportion of clinicians in the present study holding the view regarding parents' responsibility for their childrens' stuttering, it would appear to have important implications for the continuing education of speech therapists. "early intervention" the results suggest that when it came to helping preschool stutterers, 96% of respondents felt that counselling parents was the critical factor, and 76.4% felt that therapy should focus on the parents. according to onslow (1996), while counselling may not be the primary means to eliminate stuttering, counselling techniques which teach parents the correct behavioural response to stuttering represent an indispensable component in managing this disorder. according to cooper and cooper (1985a) it is an almost universally held belief of clinicians that parent counselling is the critical factor in therapy with pre-school stutterers. most clinicians (86.2%) disagreed with the statement that no matter what the age of the child, clinicians should make him/her aware of his/her stuttering behaviours. respondents agreed that clinicians should avoid using words like "stutterer" and "stuttering" when working with patients in grade one or two (62.6%). this kind of finding may represent the impact of johnson's diagnosogenic theory and does not reflect the current literature (cooper & cooper, 1985a). the majority of the sample of clinicians disagreed with the notion that children between 4-7 years of age should not be enrolled in therapy (82.1%) while 73.2% of respondents did not agree that most young stutterers will outgrow the problem without intervention. this finding supports literature which claims that as soon as a child shows signs of abnormal, persistent disfluencies, he or she should be enrolled in therapy (prins & ingham, 1983 as cited by cooper & cooper, 1985a; onslow, 1996). despite the previous findings, over 50% of respondents appeared reluctant to initiate therapy with young children as soon as their disfluent behaviour became apparent. prins and ingham (1983) suggest that this viewpoint may be the result of clinicians not wanting to draw the child's attention to his/her speech behaviours, for fear of "causing" stuttering (as cited by cooper & cooper, 1996). this fear may reduce the efficacy of therapy. although therapists felt strongly that young stutterers needed speech therapy, there was a prevailing attitude of fear about handling a young stutterer. in theory, therapists appeared to agree that young stutterers should have therapy, but they felt apprehensive about accepting this type of patient for fear of causing further harm. there was a definite contradiction between what therapists reportedly believed and how they reportedly acted. "therapy techniques" one of the strongest responses came from 96.8% of therapists who disagreed that speech therapists should avoid counselling parents of stutterers. a further 75.6% agreed with the statement that parent counselling is still a critical factor in treating adolescent stutterers. however, clinicians seemed divided as to whether or not psychotherapy for stutterers should be left to the psychologist. this finding may indicate that although clinicians are aware of the crucial role of counselling parents of stutterers, as discussed in the previous section, they do not feel adequately skilled to assume the role of psychotherapist. no specific handling techniques were probed by the questionnaire, but approaches to therapy were discussed. just over half, i.e., 51,2% of respondents disagreed with the method of getting stutterers to force themselves to speak in situations where they find fluency difficult. seventy nine (79.7) per cent of clinicians also disagreed that it was in stutterers' own interests to be called to recite in front of a class. "parents of stutterers" the majority (80.5%) of clinicians agreed that parents' misperceptions of stuttering frequently impede the child's progress. a further 58.6% of respondents believed that parents resent the speech behaviour of their stuttering child. almost 50% of the respondents did not believe that parents of stutterers tend to possess identifiably similar personality patterns. this finding is in agreement with research that has shown that the parents of stutterers are no more, nor less neurotic than parents of non-stutterers (andrews et al., 1983). "clinicians who treat stutterers" the majority (87.8%) of clinicians, reported disagreeing with the statement that clinicians are more comfortable with stutterers than with articulation disordered children. this finding is hardly surprising, as stuttering has been described as one of the least popular disorders to treat (st. louis & durrenberger, 1993). of concern, yet not unexpected, is the fact that 62.6% of respondents disagreed with the notion that most speech therapists are adept in treating stuttering. clinicians generally reported being effective in modifying the self-concept of stutterers (60.2%) while 99.2% were opposed to the idea of counselling of schoolaged stutterers not being the domain of the speech therapist. in fact, 44.7% of respondents disagreed that psychotherapy for stutterers should be left to the psychologist. in support of these findings, 98.4% of clinicians felt that counselling techniques are important skills in treating stutterers. this notion is reinforced by the results found in a previous section, namely, that counselling of parents is a very important therapy technique. this result has important implications for the training of speech therapists, as students in some training institutions often receive no formal instruction in counselling (watson, 1995). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) attitudes of a group of south african speech-language and stuttering therapy "teachers'and others'reactions to stuttering" the results suggest that the majority of clinicians surveyed believe that while teachers are generally accurate in identifying stutterers in the classroom (69.9%), they are usually unaware of the psychological ramifications of stuttering (74%), and are not knowledgeable about handling stutterers (87%). if teachers are handling stutterers inappropriately, this behaviour may result in the child's educational performance deteriorating (gottwald & starkweather, 1995). the majority of respondents (69.1%) also believed that the general public reacts more negatively to stuttering than to other speech abnormalities. "efficacy of stuttering therapy" in view of the current emphasis on intensive therapy programmes such as the successful stuttering management program (breitenfeldt and lorenz 1989) at certain academic institutions, it is of interest to note that 69.1% of respondents believed that this type of programme is the most effective way to treat stutterers. when it came to therapy, 64.2% of respondents disagreed with the statement that stuttering behaviour is relatively easy to modify. almost half (49.6%) of the respondents agreed that the type of programme followed in therapy was a significant factor in the success of therapy, and 67.5% believed that self-evaluative type therapies, or a combination of self-evaluative type therapy and operant therapy was most effective. this approach to therapy is endorsed by onslow (1996) who advocates the behavioural management of stuttering. "therapists'feelings about working with stutterers" the open-ended questions were analysed into two broad categories, according to whether respondents disliked or liked stuttering therapy, and each of these categories was further subdivided according to the themes which emerged. these themes were based on the reasons which therapists provided to explain why they liked or disliked stuttering therapy. the majority of responses fell into one of the following categories. j 1. long-term success with stutterers (or the lack thereof). 2. training (adequate or inadequate). 3. practical experience with stutterers (adequate or inadequate). ι 4. therapy programmes (unstructured or challenging). 5. stutterers' personalities (psychologically unstable or not). 6. other (no specific reason stated). it is of interest that an almost equal number of therapists fell within the "dislikes stuttering therapy" (62) category as within the "likes stuttering therapy " (61) category. of those who fell within the "dislikes stuttering therapy" category, 32.3% attributed their aversion to a lack of longterm success with stutterers. this finding is not surprising as 85.4% of the sample disagreed that stuttering is the speech disorder most amenable to therapy. this lack of success maybe attributable to what cooper and cooper (1996) referred to as the "frequency fallacy", i.e., the erroneous assumption that the number of disfluencies is the only measure of success of therapy. they claim that therapy should aim at a feeling of fluency control rather than actual fluency. however, this is only one approach and many programmes are based on attaining actual fluency. within the "dislikes stuttering therapy" group, 24.2% of respondents pathologists towards stutterers ^ expressed the view that their training was inadequate; 17.7% felt that their lack of experience with stutterers at both an undergraduate and professional level was responsible for their negative attitude towards stuttering therapy. a further 17.7% felt that the type of therapy was to blame. they maintained that stuttering therapy is often unstructured and therefore frustrating and stressful. these results suggest that an improvement in training programmes might result in a more positive outlook among clinicians on stuttering therapy. only 8.1% of the sample of therapists attributed their dislike of therapy to the individual stutterer, claiming that stutterers have severe psychological problems which speech therapy cannot address, or that they disliked stutterers. it is of interest that while 48.8% of respondents believed that stutterers have psychological problems, only 8.1% attributed their dislike of stuttering therapy to this factor. this finding may indicate that although therapists may not enjoy working with stutterers, their reasons may have little to do with their reaction to stutterers as people. on the other hand, therapists may also be reluctant to admit that their dislike of stuttering therapy stems from their view of a stutterer's personality. of the 61 respondents who claimed to "enjoy stuttering therapy", 29.5% reported feeling comfortable with stutterers for no apparent reason; others reported adequate success in therapy (19.7%) or feeling challenged and rewarded by stuttering therapy (18%). only 9.8% said that their training had adequately prepared them for treating stutterers, which highlights the need for improved training methods, while 11.5% claimed that their positive attitudes were the result of their practical experiences with stutterers. this result supports van der merwe's (1987) and leahy's (1994) findings that experience with stutterers yields more positive attitudes. a further 11.5% of therapists claimed that they only enjoyed stuttering therapy because of the counselling aspect. this finding highlights the need to incorporate counselling skills into the training of speech therapists at an undergraduate level. stereotypes of stutterers almost half of the sample (48.8%) agreed that most stutterers are likely to have psychological problems. the same percentage of respondents agreed that there are personality traits attributable to stutterers. an even greater percentage of respondents (56.1%) believed that stutterers have feelings of inferiority. these self-reported beliefs of clinicians contradict findings reported in the literature which show that stutterers are generally not psychologically abnormal, nor do they have characteristic personality traits (andrews et al., 1983). the majority of respondents (66.7%) believed that stutterers have a distorted perception of their stuttering behaviour, and 43.1% agreed that they have a distorted perception of their social relationships. over 70% of clinicians felt that stuttering was the most psychologically devastating of all speech disorders. the relationship between clinicians' attitudes regarding the personality of stutterers and their training and experience an aim of the study was to investigate whether there was any relationship between the attitudes clinicians hold about the personalities of stutterers and their training and experience. in order to investigate this aim, the results of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 questions concerning the personality attributes of stutterers were cross-tabulated with the year of graduation, years of professional experience, training approaches emphasised, and frequency of therapy with stutterers. the first area investigated was year of graduation and years of professional experience. distribution of personality stereotypes by year of graduation the majority of respondents graduated during the time span extending from 1991-1995; the minority of respondents between 1955 and 1980. due to the small number of respondents graduating during the latter period, this was treated as one category. findings represented in table 3 suggest that the time of graduation did not affect clinicians' attitudes towards stutterers. this result was supported by cross-tabulating years of professional experience with personality attributes of stutterers. the findings shown in table 2 suggest that young, newly graduated clinicians did not belinda baker, eleanor ross & joan girson differ noticeably in their attitudes towards stutterers from those who had years of professional experience. this is a surprising result as previous research (van der merwe 1987) has shown that clinicians with more experience were more optimistic about the efficacy of stuttering therapy. one might have expected younger graduates to be more progressive and liberal in their approach as they should have been exposed to more current literature on stuttering and stutterers. a possible explanation for this finding is that the greater the degree to which a group has previously been stereotyped, the less likely it is that any new information or knowledge will change the stereotype (triandis 1971). thus, stereotypes tend to persist over time (triandis 1971). white and collins (1984) provide another explanation by suggesting that the stereotype people have of stutterers is justified to some extent. they claim that when fluent speakers experience moments of disfluency, they feel shy, embarrassed and anxious. fluent speakers then logically conclude that stutterers experience these same feelings when they are disfluent, and they translate these feelings into negatable 2 distribution of personality attributes by years of experience (n = 123) personality attributes years of experience question 1 5 6 10 11 15 16 + 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems agree 27 10 10 13 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems undecided 3 4 4 5 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems disagree 21 11 . 11 4 7. most stutters have distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour agree 32 19 15 16 7. most stutters have distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour undecided 9 3 2 2 7. most stutters have distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour disagree 10 3 8 4 22. most stutterers could be described as possessing a feeling of inferiority agree 32 15 12 10 22. most stutterers could be described as possessing a feeling of inferiority undecided 9 8 7 5 22. most stutterers could be described as possessing a feeling of inferiority disagree 10 2 6 7 26. there are some personality traits characteristic of stutterers agree 25 8 14 13 26. there are some personality traits characteristic of stutterers undecided 15 8 4 6 26. there are some personality traits characteristic of stutterers disagree 11 9 7 3 30. most stutterers display a distorted perception of their own social relationships agree 21 11 11 10 30. most stutterers display a distorted perception of their own social relationships undecided 17 9 9 7 30. most stutterers display a distorted perception of their own social relationships disagree 13 5 5 5 47. stutterers are generally more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech • handicaps agree 6 0 4 2 47. stutterers are generally more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech • handicaps undecided 19 8 7 11 47. stutterers are generally more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech • handicaps disagree 26 17 14 9 / 49. clinicians must be more understanding of the feelings agree 15 9 9 7 49. clinicians must be more understanding of the feelings undecided 3 1 0 2 of stuttering clients than nonstuttering clients disagree 33 15 "16 13 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) attitudes of a group of south african speech-language pathologists towards stutterers and stuttering therapy tive personality attributes about stutterers. this explanation would account for the finding that the year of graduation or years of professional experience did not appear to influence clinicians' attitudes towards stutterers to any great extent. distribution of personality attributes by frequency of therapy with stutterers the results indicated in table 4 suggest that the frequency with which clinicians treated stutterers did not appear to shape their attitudes in general. the exception to this finding was in relation to question 3, namely clinicians who treated stutterers some of the time, most of the time, or all of the time, were more inclined to agree that stutterers have psychological problems. while most clinicians agreed that stutterers possess feelings of inferiority and have certain personality traits which characterise them, clinicians who treated stutterers all of the time were more likely to disagree with these statements. while these findings may appear to be contradictory, they reveal an interesting trend. clinicians who treat stutterers all of the time may be inclined to disagree that there are personality traits characteristic of stutterers because they have been exposed to a variety of different stutterers each of whom was unique. this finding is supported by literature which shows that clinicians who work more with stutterers have a more positive attitude towards them (st. louis & durrenberger 1993; leahy 1994) as well as by the respondents who reported a positive attitude towards stuttering therapy as a result of numerous years of experience with stutterers. conversely, clinicians who hold more positive attitudes towards stutterers might be more willing to work with them all of the time. the fact that clinicians who work with stutterers are more likely to agree that stutterers have psychological problems supports the "kernel-of-truth" hypothesis. proponents of this theory would claim that the stereotype that stutterers have psychological problems contains an element of truth. according to triandis (1971), many stereotypes origitable 3 distribution of personality stereotypes by year of graduation (n = 123) personality attributes year of graduation question 1991 1995 1986 1990 1981 1985 1955 1980 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems agree 26 8 10 16 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems undecided 3 3 5 5 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems disagree 19 11 8 9 7. most stutters have distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour agree 30 17 15 20 7. most stutters have distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour undecided 8 3 2 3 7. most stutters have distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour disagree 10 2 6 7 22. most stutterers could be agree 30 14 11 14 described as possessing feeling of inferiority a undecided 7 8 6 8 described as possessing feeling of inferiority a disagree 11 0 6 8 26. there are some personality traits chara :teristic agree 21 10 12 17 26. there are some personality traits chara :teristic undecided 15 6 5 7 of stutterers | disagree 12 6 6 6 30. most stutterers display/ a distorted perception of their own social relationships agree 18 12 9 14 30. most stutterers display/ a distorted perception of their own social relationships undecided 15 7 10 10 30. most stutterers display/ a distorted perception of their own social relationships disagree 15 3 4 6 47. stutterers are generally more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps agree 5 1 4 2 47. stutterers are generally more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps undecided 17 7 7 14 47. stutterers are generally more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps , disagree 26 14 12 14 49. clinicians must be more understanding of the feelings of stuttering clients than non1 stuttering clients agree 14 8 8 10 49. clinicians must be more understanding of the feelings of stuttering clients than non1 stuttering clients undecided 3 1 0 2 49. clinicians must be more understanding of the feelings of stuttering clients than non1 stuttering clients disagree 31 13 15 18 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 nate in this way. so, while certain stutterers may display psychological problems, these are not an integral part of the stuttering syndrome (andrews et al., 1983). the finding that clinicians who never treat stutterers or who only treat them occasionally, tend to agree with stereotypical phrases to describe stutterers, suggests that these stereotypes may result directly from clinical training, as opposed to emanating from direct contact with stutterers. distribution of personality attributes by training approach emphasised when training emphasis (theory; therapy approaches or both) was cross-tabulated with questions relating to personality attributes of stutterers, the majority of respondents fell within the "both" category. from the respondents whose training emphasised either theory or therapy, it would appear that the training emphasis did not influence attitudes to a large extent. these results must, however, be interpreted with caution as the number of respondents in belinda baker, eleanor ross & joan girson the "theory" and "therapy" cells was very small, as can be seen in table 5. distribution of personality attributes by training approach emphasised when data were cross-tabulated to compare which method of stuttering therapy was emphasised in training there appeared to be no difference in the attitudes of clinicians who were taught the "stutter-fluently" approach versus the "speak-fluently" approach, as demonstrated in table 6. again, clinicians who were taught a combined approach versus choosing a therapy depending on the patient showed no great differences in attitudes. this finding, when coupled with the results from table 5, lends credence to researchers who propose that a change in the type of training is required for student clinicians, as teaching different approaches does little to ameliorate the stereotype clinicians have of stutterers. table 4 distribution of personality attributes by frequency of therapy with stutterers (n =123) personality attributes frequency of therapy with stutterers question never once in a while some of the time most of the time all of the time 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems agree 3 19 29 5 4 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems undecided 1 5 8 2 0 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems disagree 5 22 16 1 3 7. most stutterers have a distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour agree 5 27 38 5 7 7. most stutterers have a distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour undecided 3 8 5 0 0 7. most stutterers have a distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour disagree 1 11 10 3 0 22. most stutterers could be described as possessing a feeling of inferiority agree 4 28 30 5 2 22. most stutterers could be described as possessing a feeling of inferiority undecided 3 10 14 1 1 22. most stutterers could be described as possessing a feeling of inferiority disagree 2 8 9 2 4 26. there are some personality traits characteristic of stutterers agree 3 19 31 5 2 26. there are some personality traits characteristic of stutterers undecided 4 16 9 2 2 26. there are some personality traits characteristic of stutterers disagree 2 11 13 1 3 30. most stutterers display a distorted perception of their own social relationships agree 3 20 24 4 2 30. most stutterers display a distorted perception of their own social relationships undecided 4 19 18 0 1 30. most stutterers display a distorted perception of their own social relationships disagree 2 7 11 4 4 47. stutterers generally are more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps agree 0 6 5 0 1 47. stutterers generally are more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps undecided 3 16 21 3 2 47. stutterers generally are more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps disagree 6 24 27 5 4 49. clinicians must be more agree 1 14 21 3 1 understanding of the feelings of undecided 1 1 3 1 0 stuttering clients than nonstuttering clients disagree 7 31 29 ya 6 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a t t i t u d e s of a group of south african speech-language and stuttering therapy conclusions in summary, many strongly held beliefs emerged among the therapists surveyed. some of these beliefs were reflected in the responses pertaining to the etiology of stuttering. most clinicians appeared to be in agreement that stuttering is caused by multiple co-existing factors. therapists also felt strongly about the positive role of counselling in stuttering therapy as well as about the benefits of early intervention. in direct contrast to the latter belief, therapists also mentioned experiencing extreme trepidation when approaching young stutterers and expressed the view that they might unwittingly harm the stutterer. there still appeared to be a widespread belief among clinicians that stutterers have a different psychological profile from other young children who need speech therapy, and that stutterers are more susceptible to being harmed by intervention. therapists did, however, see themselves as having a very important role in the counselling of parents of stutterers, as they believed that parents' misperceptions of stuttering frepathologists towards stutterers ^ quently impedes the child's progress. one of the most disconcerting results was that over 60% of therapists did not think that speech therapists were adept at treating stutterers and almost 50% of the sample of qualified clinicians surveyed, viewed the term "stutterer" as denoting a certain personality type as opposed to a description of a speech symptom. this belief held true irrespective of the number of years working in the field, the time of graduation, the frequency of treating stutterers, or the training emphasis. very similar results have been found in great britain and the united kingdom by cooper and rustin (1985). the results are therefore not uniquely south african. nevertheless, the findings have important implications for training and future research. implications for student training and continuing education • negative attitudes may reduce the effectiveness of stuttering therapy (lass et al., 1989). it is therefore necespersonality attributes training approach emphasised question theory therapy approaches both 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems agree 7 . 0 53 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems undecided 0 1 15 3. chances are that most stutterers have psychological problems disagree 8 1 38 7. most stutterers have a distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour agree 11 1 70 7. most stutterers have a distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour undecided 3 0 13 7. most stutterers have a distorted perception of their own stuttering behaviour disagree 1 1 23 22. most stutterers could be described as possessing a agree 11 1 57 22. most stutterers could be described as possessing a undecided 1 1 27 feeling of inferiority , disagree 3 0 22 26. there are some personality traits char icteristic agree 8 0 52 26. there are some personality traits char icteristic undecided 6 1 26 of stutterers | disagree 1 1 28 30. most stutterers display s distorted perception of their own social relationships agree 5 1 47 30. most stutterers display s distorted perception of their own social relationships undecided 4 1 37 30. most stutterers display s distorted perception of their own social relationships disagree 6 0 22 47. stutterers generally are more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps agree 0 0 12 47. stutterers generally are more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps undecided 10 0 35 47. stutterers generally are more intelligent than those with other kinds of speech handicaps disagree 5 2 59 49. clinicians must be more understanding of the feelings of stuttering clients than non1 stuttering clients agree 3 0 37 49. clinicians must be more understanding of the feelings of stuttering clients than non1 stuttering clients undecided 1 0 5 49. clinicians must be more understanding of the feelings of stuttering clients than non1 stuttering clients disagree 11 2 64 table 5 distribution of personality attributes by training approach emphasised (n = 123) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 belinda baker, eleanor ross & joan girson sary for educators to increase the clinician's awareness of areas in which they feel secure as well as the areas of insecurity as "the more realistic clinicians are about themselves, the more able they are to respond positively and constructively to their clients" (gregory, 1982: 17). • open discussions should be held during the training of speech therapists on the issue of stereotypes. lass et al., (1989) believe that this approach may reduce the negative impact of holding stereotypes. • van riper (1977) advocated the training of specialists in the field of stuttering in an attempt to improve speech therapists' attitudes towards stuttering. the fact that 98.4% of respondents agreed that clinicians working with stutterers need to be skilled in counselling techniques may strengthen the case for training fluency specialists (cooper & cooper, 1996). these specialists could receive special training, not only in fluency disorders, but in counselling techniques (cooper & cooper, 1985a). • specialized training beyond initial qualifications may also overcome the problem of a lack of practical experience with stutterers at an undergraduate level. this area which needs to be addressed as 17.7% of the present sample of south african speech therapists who disliked stuttering therapy claimed that the reason for their attitudes stemmed from a lack of practical experience. • competency-based educational strategies may overcome the negative attitudes of speech therapists working with stutterers. if a list of specific competencies was incorporated into clinical training, clinicians could graduate with a basic foundation to which additional skills and qualifications could be added (culatta & harris, 1976). this system is advantageous for tutors as well, as it is likely to be easier to provide specific feedback and suggestions for improvements when one is working from a specific list of competencies (culatta & harris, 1976). • within a south african context where resources are limited, group therapy experiences in treating stutterers during training may be a realistic solution. these expetable 6 distribution of personality attributes by training approach emphasised (n = 123) personality attributes approach emphasised question stutter fluently speak fluently combined depends on patient 3. chances are that most agree 10 7 26 17 stutterers have psychological undecided 3 3 6 4 problems disagree 10 10 18 9 7. most stutters have distorted agree 14 15 35 18 perception of their own undecided 3 2 7 4 stuttering behaviour disagree 6 3 8 8 22. most stutterers could be agree 8 10 31 20 described as possessing a undecided 6 6 14 3 feeling of inferiority disagree 9 4 5 7 26. there are some agree 9 10 29 12 1 personality traits characteristic undecided 8 5 11 9 : of stutterers disagree 6 5 10 9 1 30. most stutterers display agree 12 7 23 11 ! a distorted perception of their undecided 7 8 15 12 1 own social relationships disagree 4 5 12 7 47. stutterers are generally agree 1 1 10 0 more intelligent than those undecided 11 7 15 12 with other kinds of speech • handicaps disagree 11 12 25 18 / 49. clinicians must be more agree 5 5 21 9 understanding of the feelings undecided 1 3 1 1 of stuttering clients than nonstuttering clients disagree 17 12 2̂8 20 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) attitudes of a g r o u p of south african speech-language and stuttering therapy riences have been found to effectively reduce the negative stereotypes that clinicians hold in respect of stutterers (leahy, 1994). this strategy may also enable more student clinicians to treat stutterers at an undergraduate level (mallard et al., 1988). • the need for continuing education in stuttering is evidenced by the response of over half the sample who disagreed with the view that they currently have at their disposal, adequate therapy techniques with which to treat stutterers. implications for further research • due to time constraints, it was beyond the scope of the present study to consider more of the possible contributing factors in training which may influence clinicians' attitudes. o f particular interest may be to ascertain whether there is a difference in attitudes between speechlanguage therapists who were exposed to intensive group training at an undergraduate level with those who were not. it would be of interest to see if results supported leahy's (1994) findings that group therapy yielded more positive attitudes. • an interesting area of research would be to attempt to consciously ameliorate the stereotype which undergraduate students may have of stutterers. in conclusion, it is hoped that this study m a y raise the awareness of clinicians and educators regarding the prevailing attitudes which exist with regard to stutterers. holding on to certain attitudes may cause clinicians to neglect their clients' individual attitudinal and behavioural responses, and may impede therapy. if clinicians feel that they are not competent in handling stutterers, they may look for convenient ways to circumvent their lack of confidence. onslow (1996) believes that australian therapists may have adopted this strategy by referring stutterers to intensive treatment centres and/or programmes, instead of handling them themselves. the same may hold true in south africa, as many of the respondents in this survey specifically mentioned referring patients to breitenfeldt and lorenz's (1989) successful stuttering management program. this tendency may, as onslow (1996) believes, perpetuate a cycle of ignorance as therapists deprive themselves of personal experience in treating stutterers. a way to break this cycle may be through workshops for therapists who wish to learn the latest therapy techniques for stutterers, j r e f e r e n c e s andrews, g., hoddinott, s., craig, α., howie, p., feyer, α., & neilson, m. (1983). stuttering: a review of research findings and theories circa 1982. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 48, 226 246. barbosa, l., schiefer, α., & chiari, b. (1995). how students of speech therapy perceive stuttering: a matter of prejudice. proceedings: first world congress on fluency disorders, munich, 1994. breitenfeldt, d.h. and lorenz, d.r. (1989). successful stuttering management program, eastern washington university, cheney, washington. cooper, e. (1996): personal communication. cooper, e.b. & cooper, c.s. (1985a). cooper personalised fluency control therapy revised. allen, tx: dlm. cooper, e.b. & cooper, c.s. (1985b). clinicians'attitudes towards stuttering: a decade of change (1973 -1983). journal of fluency disorders, 10, 19 33. 1 cooper, e.b. & cooper, c.s. (1996 in press). clinicians' attitudes pathologists towards stutterers z o towards stuttering: two decades of change. journal of fluency disorders. cooper, e.b. & rustin, l. (1985). clinicians' attitudes towards stuttering in the united states and great britain: a cross cultural study. journal of fluency disorders, 10, 1 17. culatta, r. & harris, a. (1976). a competency-based system for the initial training of speech pathologists. asha, 18, 10, 733 738. daly, d.a., simon, c.a. & burnett-stolnack, m. (1995). helping adolescents who stutter focus on fluency. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 26, 2, 162 168. de vaus, d.a. (1991). surveys in social research, london, allen & unwin. emerick, l.l. (1960). extensional definition and attitude toward stuttering. journal of speech and hearing research, 3 , 2 , 181 185. gottwald & starkweather c.w. (1995). fluency intervention for preschoolers, and their families in the public school language, speech and hearing services in schools, 26, 117 127. gregory, h.h. (1982). the clinician's attitudes. in gruss, j. (ed) counselling stutterers. memphis, t.n.: speech foundation of america. ham, r.e. (1990a). what is stuttering: variations and stereotypes. journal of fluency disorders, 15, 259 273^ jones, r.a. (1977). self-fulfilling prophecies social, psychological, and physiological effects of expectancies. new jersey: lawrence erlbaum associates publishers. kalinowski, j.s., lerman, j.w., & watt, j. (1987). a preliminary examination of the perceptions of self and others in stutterers and non-stutterers. journal of fluency disorders, 12, 317 331. lass, n.j., ruscello, d.m., schmitt, j.f. , pannbacker, m.d., orlando, m.b. , dean, k.a., ruziska, j.c. & bradshaw, k.h. (1992). teachers' perceptions of stutterers. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 23, 78 -81. leahy, m. (1994). attempting to ameliorate student therapists' negative stereotype of the stutterers. european journal of disorders of communication, 29, 39 49. leith, (1971) clinical training in stuttering therapy : a survey asha, 13, 1, 6 8. mallard, α., gardner, l. & downey, c. (1988). clinical training in stuttering for school clinicians. journal of fluency disorders, 13, 253 259. onslow, μ (1996). behavioural management of stuttering, san diego: singular publishing group. peters, t. & guitar, b. (1991). stuttering :an integrated approach to its nature and treatment, baltimore, md: williams and wilkins. rustin, l. & cook, f. (1995). parental involvement in the treatment of stuttering. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 26, 2, 127 137. starkweather, c.w. (1982). talking with parents of young stutterers. in gruss, j (ed) counselling stutterers. memphis, t.n.: speech foundation of america. st. louis, k.o. & durrenberger, c.h. (1993). what communication disorders do experienced clinicians prefer to manage? asha, 35, 12, 23 31. st. louis, k. & lass, n. (1981). a survey of communicative disorders students' attitudes toward stuttering. journal of fluency disorders, 6, 49 79. triandis, h.c. (1971). attitude and attitude change. u.s.a.: john wiley and sons inc. turnbaugh, k., guitar, b. & hoffman, p. (1979). speech clinicians' attribution of personality traits as a function of stuttering severity. journal of speech and hearing research, 22, 37 45. van der merwe, k. (1987). the attitudes of south african speech clinicians towards stuttering. university of cape town: unpublished research report. van riper, c. (1977). the public school specialist in stuttering. asha, 19, 7, 467 469. watson, j.b. (1995). exploring the attitudes of adults who stutter. journal of communication disorders, 28, 143 164. woods, c.l. & williams, d.e. (1971). speech clinician's conceptions of boys and men who stutter. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 36, 225 235. woods, c.l. & williams, d.e. (1976). traits attributed to stuttering and normally fluent males. journal of speech and hearing research, 19, 267 278. yairi, e. & williams, d. (1970). speech clinician's stereotypes of elementary schoolboys who stutter. journal of communication disorders, 3, 161 170. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) municatioi at telkom, we believe that everyone has the right to better communication. that's why we've developed the teldem a portable terminal designed for the deaf. thanks to teldem, your business is your own you no longer need to rely on a third party for telephone messages. face to face communication is easier too, since teldem replaces a paper and pen. at only r1 1 .40 (including vat) a month, teldem is much cheaper than imported communication terminals. and of course, you get all the benefits of telkom's service and maintenance facility. for more information, ask a friend to call this toll-free number: 0 8 0 0 11 95 96 w telkom teldem r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie a. van der merwe, d.phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit von pretoria. i.c. uys, d. phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria. j.m. loots, dsc (fisiologie) (pretoria) departement menslike bewegingskunde, universiteit van pretoria. r.j. grimbeek, bsc (hons.) (pretoria) departement statistiek universiteit van pretoria. opsomming die rol van kontekstuele faktore by verbale apraksie is belangrik omdat dit implikasies inhou vir die interpretasie van simptome, die aard van die afwyking en vir verdere navorsing. die invloed van die kontekstuele faktore, klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe op die ouditief waarneembare simptome van vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en een persoon met verbale.ontwikkelingsapraksie is nagegaan. die effek van sistematiese variasie in die klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe van onsineenhede op die tipe en aantal foute in die spraak van die verbaal apraktiese sprekers is bepaal. daar is bevind dat afwykings in temporale vloei, klankdistorsie en afwykings in prosodie in hierdie volgorde die hoogste frekwensie van voorkoms vertoon ongeag die klankstruktuur of artikulasieeienskappe van die uiting. daar is wel 'n toename in klankvervangings, weglatings en byvoegings namate die moeilikheidsgraad van 'n uiting toeneem. dit wil voorkom asof die simptome van verbale apraksie op grond van konteks-sensitiwiteit verdeel kan word in kernsimptome en geassosieerde simptome. abstract the role of contextual factors in verbal apraxia is important as it has implications for the interpretation of behaviour, the nature of the disorder as well as for further research. the influence of the contextual factors, sound structure and articulatory features on the auditorily perceived symptoms of four patients with acquired verbal apraxia and one patient with developmental verbal apraxia, was investigated. the effect of systematic variation in the sound structure and articulatory features of nonsense units on the type and number of mistakes in the speech of the apraxic speakers, was determined. findings indicate that abnormalities in temporal flow, sound distortion and abnormalities in prosody exhibited in this order the highest frequency of occurrence, independent of the sound structure or articulatory features of the utterance. there is, however, an escalation of sound substitutions, ofnissions and additions with increasing complexity of the utterance. the symptoms of apraxia have been classified in core symptoms and associated symptoms based on their context sensitivity. verbale apraksie is vir meer as 120 jaar reeds 'n omstrede onderwerp. oor die kenmerkende eienskappe, die onderliggende aard van die afwyking en ook die naam van die afwyking het sterk meningsverskil ontstaan (rosenbek, kent & la pointe, 1984). broca het in 1861 die afwyking as "aphemia" benoem en sedertdien is talle ander name daaraan.gegee (johns & la pointe, 1976; darley, aronson & brown, 1975). groot verwarring het ontstaan en die term verbale apraksie is gebruik om verskillende tipes gedrag by verskillende tipes pasiente te beskryf (buckingham, 1981). dit is opvallend dat weinig van die vroee navorsers die afwyking wat hul bestudeer het, gedefinieer het. darley (1967) was die eerste resente navorser wat verbale apraksie gedefinieer en as 'n afwyking in spraakprogrammering beskryf het. darley het sodoende rigting gegee aan 'n groep navorsers wat vandag steeds hierdie afwyking bestudeer. 'n groot mate van eenstemmigheid is die afgelope paar jaar bereik. verbale apraksie word tans algemeen as 'n afwyking in spraakprogrammering beskryf en dit word onderskei van broca-afasie, fonemiese parafasie en disartrie wat ook afwykings in spraakproduksie weens breinskade is (mohr, pessin, finkelstein, funkenstein, duncan & davis', 1978;' buckingham, 1981; itoh & sasanuma, 1984; mackenzie, 1982). die kenmerkende eienskappe van suiwer verbale apraksie word deur kent & rosenbek (1983) beskryf as onvlot spraakproduksie met probeer-en-tref-artikulasiebewegings, pogings tot selfkorreksie, onkonstantheid in die foute by herhaalde produksie van dieselfde woord en disprosodie. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 © sasha 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie 11 ten spyte van die eenstemmigheid wat bereik is, bestaan daar nog talle onbeantwoorde vrae oor verbale apraksie. rosenbek et al. (1984) verwys na "questions in search of answers" en noem onder meer die volgende: is die huidige definisie voldoende? is dit 'n afwyking op die fonologiese vlak of slegs versteurde artikulasie? is dit 'n dipe disartrie? wat is willekeurige beweging? wat is foutonkonstantheid? daar is 'n groeiende bewustheid dat navorsing oor verbale apraksie gerig moet word deur 'n volledige teoretiese model (itoh & sasanuma, 1984; rosenbek et al. 1984; kelso & tuller, 1981; marquardt & sussman, 1984). slegs enkele teoretiese modelle is reeds toegepas op liggaamsen verbale apraksie (roy, 1978; russell & whitaker, 1979; itoh & sasanuma, 1984; kelso & tuller, 1981; van der merwe, 1986). 'n model met groot moontlikhede vir verdere toepassing op verbale apraksie is waarskynlik die koalisiemodel (kelso & tuller, 1981; kelso, tuller & harris, 1983; tuller, fitch & turvey, 1982). die koalisiemodel is 'n teoretiese model wat gebaseer is op die idee dat gedrag ontstaan uit die funksionele koalisie van strukture ("coordinative structures"). tydelike koalisie of samewerking tussen neurale strukture en ook anatomiese strukture soos die spiere, ontstaan afhangende van die beweging wat uitgevoer moet word. die beweging of gedrag wat uitgevoer word, word in hierdie teorie omskryf as die konteks waarbinne funksionering plaasvind. hierdie teorie verklaar volgens die aanhangers daarvan die wyse waarop die individu die vryhede van die stelsel hanteer. kelso & tuller (1981) noem dit 'n "theory of context and constraints rather than a theory of commands". die konteks-sensitiwiteit van gedrag is dus die belangrikste eienskap van die koalisiemodel en afwykings in gedrag word beskou as die gevolg van 'n afwyking of onvermoe tot 'n koalisie van sekere strukture. kelso en tuller (1981) pas die koalisiemodel toe op apraksie. die toepassing op liggaamsapraksie is meer volledig as die op^verbale apraksie. die toepassing sentreer hoofsaaklik om die invloed van konteks, wat benut behoort te word in die ' i verklaring van apraksie. geen verduideliking van konteks in spraakproduksie word egter verskaf nie. slegs die bekende voorbeeld van 'n diskrepansie tussen proposisionele en outomatiese spraakproduksie word genoem. ten einde die koalisiemodel op verbale apraksie toe te pas, is dit dus nodig om kontekstuele faktore by spraakproduksie te identifiseer. i die huidige studie is uitgevoer binne die raamwerk van 'n omvattende spraakproduksiemodel wat spraakproduksie hierargies voorstel, maar ook die konsep van die kontekssensitiwiteit van spraak inkorporeer (van der merwe, 1986). die koalisiemodel word gewoonlik direk in teenstelling met die hierargiese model van kontrole gestel (kelso & tuller, 1981). die konsep van konteks-sensitiwiteit'kan egter wel bevredigend met die hierargiese model versoen word. sekere neurale dele verrig waarskynlik "hoer" funksies as ander dele, maar tree in 'n funksionele koalisie wat kan verander na gelang van die konteks waarin gedrag plaasvind. enkele kontekstuele faktore in spraakproduksie is hipoteties ge'identifiseer in hierdie model (van der merwe, 1986) en sluit die volgende in: willekeurigheid van inisiering, klankstruktuur, motoriese kompleksiteit, lengte van die uiting, bekendheid en spoed van produksie. twee van die belangrikste van hierdie kontekstuele faktore, wat ook sistematies gevarieer kan word, is klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe. die identifikasie van hierdie twee eienskappe van spraak as kontekstuele faktore kan teoreties gemotiveer word. daar is aanduidings in die literatuur dat sekere spraakklanke en kombinasies van klanke in woorde, groter vaardigheid vereis en moeiliker is om te bemeester as ander (calvert, 1980; oiler & macneilage, 1983). die feit dat sekere spraakklanke vroeer aangeleer word deur die kind en dat die eerste uitings wat geproduseer word, bestaan uit 'n konsonant (k) en 'n vokaal (v) dus kv of 'n reduplikasie daarvan (kvkv) (ingram, 1976), kan daarop dui dat die eerste klanke wat aangeleer word en die kvkv-struktuur makliker is om te produseer as ander klanke en strukture. dit is egter moeilik om die moeilikheidsgraad van 'n uiting toe te skryf aan 'n spesifieke faktor. 'n sekere kombinasie van klanke kan op 'n fonologiese enkoderingsvlak moeiliker wees, dit kan hoer eise stel aan die geheue van die persoon en dit kan motories moeiliker wees om te produseer. daar is faktore wat motoriese kompleksiteit kan verhoog sonder dat die klankstruktuur verander. hierdie faktore sluit koartikulasiemoontlikhede en groter variasie in artikulasie-eienskappe soos die opeenvolgende strekking en ronding van die lippe in. daar is weinig gegewens oor motoriese kompleksiteit in die literatuur en bogenoemde voorbeelde is slegs hipoteties. die totale konsep van konteks-sensitiwiteit soos hier uiteengesit, is nuut, hipoteties en onontwikkeld. die moontlikheid bestaan egter wel dat sekere uitings vanwee hul besondere klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe verskillende eise stel aan 'n spreker en dus as verskille in konteks gereken kan word. daar is aanduidings in die literatuur dat variasie in sekere van hierdie faktore 'n effek het op verbale apraksiesimptome (kent & rosenbek, 1983; collins, rosenbek & wertz, 1983; wertz, la pointe & rosenbek, 1984), maar in geen vorige ondersoek is faktore soos klankstruktuur, artikulasie-eienskappe en koartikulasiemoontlikhede sistematies gevarieer nie. indien variasie in hierdie faktore 'n effek het op die waargenome simptome van verbale apraksie, hou die feit belangrike implikasies in vir die interpretasie van simptome, die aard van die afwyking en vir verdere navorsing. dit sal ook deels 'n verklaring bied vir die bekende veranderlikheid van verbale apraksie (wertz, la pointe & rosenbek, 1984; kent & rosenbek, 1983; collins, rosenbek & wertz, 1983). dit is moontlik dat sekere aspekte van veranderlikheid deur kontekstuele invloede veroorsaak word. die invloed van sistematiese variasie in die kontekstuele faktore, klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe, op die spraakproduksie van die verbaal apraktiese spreker word dus in hierdie ondersoek nagegaan. daar sal bepaal word of daar enige verband bestaan tussen die eienskappe van 'n uiting en die ouditief waarneembare foute. hierdie ondersoek is deel van 'n groter ondersoek waarin ook akoestiese analises uitgevoer is ten einde die invloed van kontekstuele faktore te bepaal (van der merwe, 1986). metode: doel: die hoofdoel van die ondersoek is om te bepaal of sekere kontekstuele faktore in spraakproduksie, naamlik die klankstruktuur (foneemstruktuur) en die artikulasie-eienskappe die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 a. van der merwe, i.c. uys, j.m. loots, r.j. grimbeek (motoriese kompleksiteit) van 'n uiting 'n effek het op die frekwensie en tipe ouditief waarneembare simptome of foute in die spraak van persone met verbale apraksie. proefpersone: -* die proefpersone het bestaan uit vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie en een persoon met 'n verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie. die kriterium is gestel dat 'n suiwer verbale apraksie vertoon moet word. vir die vier persone met verworwe verbale apraksie is die kriteria van kent en rosenbek (1983) gestel. hierdie persone het onvlot spraakproduksie met probeer-en-tref-artikulasiebewegings, pogings tot selfkorreksie, onkonstantheid in die foute by herhaalde produksie van dieselfde woord en disprosodie vertoon. ten einde 'n gepaardgaande afasie te beperk moes die proefpersone in staat wees om: — verbaal te kommunikeer met geen uitgesproke agrammatisme nie; — sinne te kan formuleer met 'n stel gegewe woorde; — 'n stel gegewe sinne te kan lees; en — normale taalbegrip te vertoon soos bepaal met 'n bekende afasietoets. die teenwoordigheid van 'n gepaardgaande disartrie is ook uitgeskakel. die proefpersone moes: — geen waarneembare boonste motorneurongesigof -tongparese vertoon nie; — geen ander neuromotoriese simptome in die liggaam vertoon as 'n hemiparese van die arm en been nie; en — geen disartriesimptome soos vegetatiewe probleme of 'n konstante distorsie vertoon nie. die lokalisasie van die letsel is nie gekontroleer in hierdie ondersoek nie omdat daar sterk aanduidings in die literatuur is dat daar nie 'n direkte verband tussen die lokalisasie van die letsel en die simptome van verbale apraksie bestaan nie (marquardt & sussman, 1984). die jongste beskikbare proefpersone is geselekteer. drie van die proefpersone is egter ouer as 65 jaar. ten einde die moontlike effek van ouderdom na te gaan, is daar ook afgepaarde kontrolepersone by die studie ingesluit. die resultate van die kontrolepersone word egter nie vir hierdie aspek van die studie gebruik nie omdat geen van die ouditief waarneembare foute by die kontrolepersone voorkom nie. daar kan dus aanvaar word dat die ouderdom van die proefpersone nie 'n effek gehad het op die resultate nie. die vyfde proefpersoon is op die ouderdom van vier as 'n geval met verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie gediagnoseer op grond van die voorkoms van die volgende simptome: — 'n diskrepansie tussen ekspressiewe vermoe en reseptiewe taalvermoe wat nie meer as ses maande agter sy chronologiese ouderdom is nie. — 'n on vermoe om willekeurig spraakbewegings uit te voer met gevolglike nie-verbaliteit. — kommunikasie het deur middel van gebare geskied. ten tye van die ondersoek het hy reeds ses jaar lank verbale apraksieterapie (van der merwe, 1976; 1985) ontvang en gebruik vyf tot seswoordsinne. gegewens omtrent die proefpersone word aangegee in tabel 1. luisteraars: twee luisteraars is geselekteer om deel te neem aan die ouditiewe analise. die een het opgetree as hoofluisteraar tesame met die ondersoeker en die ander as kontroleur-luisteraar. die hoofluisteraar is 'n spraakterapeut met 'n meestersgraad en met 'n paar jaar ondervinding in sprekerherkenning vir forensiese doeleindes. die kontroleur-luisteraar is 'n fonetikus verbonde aan 'n universiteit. beide is dus hoogs gesofistikeerde luisteraars. materiaal: ten einde die invloed van variasie in die klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe van 'n uiting op die ouditief waarneembare simptome na te gaan, is eenhede ontwikkel waarvan die klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe volkome gekontroleerd is. onsineenhede is saamgestel omdat die eienskappe van betekenisvolle woorde nie volkome kontroleerbaar en dus vergelykbaar is nie. die eenhede is in afrikaans en engels saamgestel en op so 'n wyse dat presies dieselfde klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe daarin voorkom (kyk tabelle 2 en 3). die samestelling van die materiaal word omvattend bespreek in van der merwe (1986) en slegs die belangrikste kriteria word hier genoem. wat die klankstruktuur van die eenhede betref, is vyf struktuurgroepe geselekteer. die klankstruktuurgroepe (s) sluit dupliserings van die kv-sillabe (wat vroeg voorkom in kinderspraak, moontlik die eenheid van spraakproduksie is en moontlik maklik is om te produseer) met meer en minder variasie in die aantal klanke wat gebruik word, (si en s2) en ook klankstruktuurgroepe wat die kvken kv-sillabes en uitbreidings daarvan (s3, 4 en 5) bevat, in. baie ander strukture kan gebruik word, maar daar is hiermee volstaan vanwee die groot hoeveelheid data wat dit reeds verskaf. die vyf klankstruktuurgroepe wat ingesluit is, is die volgende: 1 struktuurgroep 1: κι vi k1 v2 struktuurgroep 2: κι vi k2 v2 struktuurgroep 3: κι vi k2 v2 k3 struktuurgroep 4: κι vi k2 struktuurgroep 5: κι vi k2 v2 k3 v3 k4 en κι vi k2 v2 k3 v2 k4 die syfers wat hier aangegee word, verwys na die getal verskillende konsonante of vokale wat in 'n spesifieke eenheid voorkom. die eenhede bevat bekende foneme en voldoen aan die fonotaktiese reels van afrikaans en engels. hierdie proses van produksie is dus verwant aan spraakproduksie van betekenisvolle woorde. die artikulasie-eienskappe van die foneme in die eenhede is the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a. van der merwe, i.c. uys, j.m. loots, r.j. grimbeek 13 tabel 1: gegewens omtrent die proefpersone kriteria proefpersone kriteria 1 2 3 4 5 oorsake svi* svi svi svi verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie tydperk sedert aanvang van toestand 6 maande + 6 maande + 1 jaar + 1 jaar + geboorte ouderdom 30 jaar 70 jaar 66 jaar 70 jaar 10 jaar geslag vroulik vroulik vroulik manlik manlik spreektaal engels engels engels afrikaans afrikaans gepaardgaande afasie geen minimaal minimaal minimaal geen behandeling verbale-apraksie-terapie verbale-apraksie-terapie verbale-apraksie-terapie verbale-apraksie-terapie verbale-apraksie-terapie taalbegrip normaal gering ingekort normaal normaal normaal volgens chronologiese ouderdom op reynell-skaal ander probleme geen geen geen matige hoe frekwensie gehoorverlies gering verstandelik vertraag (i.k. 81 + ) lokalisasie van die letsel links frontotemporaal onbekend onbekend links parietaal perifeer en strek tot by posterior been van kapsula interna onbekend * svi serebrovaskulere insident gekontroleer met betrekking tot die plek en wyse van artikulasie. die eise wat die produksie van die eenhede aan die spreker stel ten opsigte van adaptasie by die klankomgewing en koartikulasiemoontlikhede (borden & harris, 1984) is in aanmerking geneem by die keuse van klanke wat in die eenhede voorkom. die moontlikheid bestaan dat eenhede wat oor verskillende artikulasie-eienskappe beskik, verskil in motoriese kompleksiteit en dus as verskille in konteks gereken kan word. j ι vier artikulasie-eienskapgr0epe (a) is onderskei (kyk tabelle 2 en 3). al die uitings in a1 en a2 begin met ibl wat 'n mate van adaptasie by die klankomgewing toelaat, maar antisiperende koartikulasie verhoed (borden & harris, 1984). die ibl staan by al voor 'n vokaal wat min lipbeweging vereis en by a2 voor 'n vokaal wat lipronding vereis. die tweede vokaal is by al en a2 afwisselend gestrek en gerond. die tweede konsonant by al en a2 is die itl wat antisiperende koartikulasie toelaat (borden en harris, 1984) vir die tweede vokaal. by s3, s4 en s5 is die lil die derde konsonant en hierdie konsonant vereis spesifieke aanpassing in lipbeweging. wat die artikulasie-eienskappe van a3 en a4 betref, is dieselfde tipe kontrole toegepas. al die uitings in a3 en a4 begin met idl wat die lippe en in 'n mate die mandibula vrylaat vir antisiperende koartikulasie (borden & harris, 1984). die antisiperende koartikulasie-moontlikhede in hierdie twee groepe is dus hoer as by al en a2. die eerste en tweede vokale is soos by al en a2 gekontroleer met betrekking tot gerondheid en strekking. die tweede konsonant ikl laat ook soos itl 'n mate van antisiperende koartikulasie van die lippe toe. die lil word ook om dieselfde redes as by a1 en a2, by a3 en a4 gebruik. die feit dat die eienskappe van die eenhede sistematies gevarieer is, maak dit moontlik om die invloed van hierdie eienskappe op spraakfoute goed gekontroleerd na te gaan. die proefpersone het elke eenheid vier keer geproduseer. 'n totaal van 160 uitings per persoon is dus versamel. herhaalde uitings van dieselfde eenheid is verkry ten einde te kontroleer vir die veranderlikheid van verbale apraksie. die spraakopnames is in 'n taallaboratorium gedoen en sluit die volgende apparaat in: — 'n nakamichi 550-bandopnemer; — 'n beyer dynamic m201-mikrofoon. die apparate wat gebruik is vir die ouditiewe analise was ook van die taallaboratorium van die universiteit van pretoria en het bestaan uit: — die tandberg tcr 5600 kassetspeler; — die tandberg learning laboratory met 'n is 9-luidspreker. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34,1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 a. van der merwe, i.c. uys, j.m. loots, r.j. grimbeek tabel 2: eenhede waarvan die klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe gekontroleer is (afrikaans) klankstruk= tuurgroepe (s) artikulasie-eienskapgroepe (a) klankstruk= tuurgroepe (s) 1 2 3 4 ] 1.1 babe 1.2 bobs 1.3 dade 1.4 dode 1.5 babu 1.6 bobu 1.7 dadu 1.8 dodu 2 2.1 b a t s 2.2 bote 2.3 daks 2.4 doks 2.5 batu 2.6 botu 2.7 daku 2.8 doku 3 3.1 b a t s f 3.2 b o t s f 3.3 d a k e f 3.4 d o k s f 3.5 b a t u f 3.6 b o t u f 3.7 dakuf 3.8 dokuf 4 4.1 b a t 4.2 b o t 4.3 dak 4.4 dok 4.5 b e t 4.6 but 4.7 dek 4.8 du:k 5 5 . ] b a t e f u p 5.2 b o t e f u p 5.3 dakefup 5.4 doksfup 5 . 5 b a t u f u p 5.6 botufup 5.7 dakufup 5.8 dokufup tabel 3: eenhede waarvan die klankstruktuur en artikulasie-eienskappe gekontroleer is (engels) klankstruk= tuurgroepe (s) artikulasie-eienskapgroepe (a) klankstruk= tuurgroepe (s) 1 2 3 4 1 1.1 ba : b a i 1.2 bob® l . 3 d q : d a e 1.4 dcd® 1.5 b a : b u 1.6 t d b u 1.7 da:du 1.8 dcdu 2 2.1 ba:iffi 2.2 bd"i® 2.3 da:ka? 2.4 duka; 2.5 batu 2.6 b o i u 2.7 d a : k u 2.8 dcku 3 3.1 ba lisef 3.2 boiarf 3.3da:ka>f 3.4 ddkaif h h 3.5 b a : t u f 3.6 b c t u f 3 . 7 d a : k u f 3.8ddkuf 4 4.1 b a : t h 4 . 2 b o i 4 . 3 d a : k 4.4 dck h h 4.5 bffit 4.6 but 4.7 dsek 4.8 du:k 5 5 . 1 b a : i a ; f u p 5 . 2 bdtha?fup 5 . 3 da:k£efup 5.4 ddkffifup 5.5 b a : i u f u p 5 . 6 bdtilifup 5 . 7 d a : k u f u p 5 . 8 dnkufup the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie 15 prosedure: — die materiaal is in geskrewe vorm (elke eenheid afsonderlik op 'n kaartjie in groot drukskrif) voorgehou en die ondersoeker het die uiting twee tot drie keer herhaal sodat die spreker 'n duidelike beeld van die eenheid kon verkry. sodoende is probleme as gevolg van 'n onvermoe om die eenheid te onthou, sover as moontlik uitgeskakel. — die prosedure van kent en rosenbek (1983) is gevolg, naamlik om aan die proefpersoon die geleentheid te gee om die eenheid 'n paar keer te oefen met die hulp van die ondersoeker. sonder die oefengeleentheid sou die proefpersone soms nie in staat gewees het om die eenheid te produseer nie. sodra die proefpersoon dit eenkeer "korrek" geproduseer het, is hy/sy gestop, die instruksie is herhaal en die opname het plaasgevind. die proefpersoon het die uiting dan vier keer herhaal sonder enige verdere hulp. die prosedure het dus meegebring dat die uitings selfge'inisieerd was. — geen instruksies omtrent spraakspoed is aan die proefpersone gegee nie. sodoende is nie ingemeng met spontane aanpassings in spoed nie. die normale sprekers is wel aangese om net 'n normale spraakspoed te handhaaf. — die volgorde waarin die eenhede aangebied is, was dieselfde by alle persone. dit is gekontroleer ten einde te verhoed dat eenhede met soortgelyke eienskappe na mekaar geproduseer word en adaptasie intree. data-analise: in die eerste stadium van die analise is 'n lys saamgestel van alle spraakfoute wat voorgekom het by die vyf proefpersone. foutkategoriee is dus bepaal. die twee luisteraars en die ondersoeker het deelgeneem'hieraan. in die tweede stadium het,die hoofluisteraar en ondersoeker gesamentlik na die spraakopnames geluister en idie foute in elke uiting aangeteken. daar is gemiddeld tienjkeer na elke uiting geluister en vyftien tot twintig uur is aan die analise van elke spreker se opnames bestee. die kontroleur-luisteraar het op 'n later stadium steekproewe gedoen en ook na alle uitings geluister waar ooreenstemming nie bereik is nie. die beoordelings van die kontroleur-luisteraar het honderd persent met beoordelings van die twee luisteraars ooreengekom. sy beslissings in probleemgevalle is aanvaar. sewe tipes foute het voorgekom by die verbaal apraktiese sprekers. elk van hieidie foutkategoriee (a-g) het bestaan uit 'n aantal subkategoriee wat volledig aangegee word in die bylae. die foutkategoriee is die volgende: a. vervangings b. verandering van die struktuur van die eenheid c. klankdistorsie d. afwykings in temporale vloei e. afwykings in vlotheid f. spraakfoute gevolg deur selfkorreksie g. afwykings in prosodie daar is gepoog om die groeperings beskrywend te maak en om nie oorsaaklike aanduidings te gee nie. dit is onmoontlik om in hierdie stadium oorsake te identifiseer. die gebruik van oorsaaklike terme sal interpretasie van die data waarskynlik ook be'invloed en objektiwiteit benadeel. enkele probleme het wel voorgekom in die groepering van foute. veranderinge in die plek van lettergreepafbreking en afbreking binne 'n geslote lettergreep is as veranderinge in die struktuur van die eenheid geklassifiseer. die enigste ander moontlike klassifikasie is by die probleme in temporale vloei. dit was egter nie bloot 'n probleem in hierdie opsig nie, omdat onsensitiwiteit vir lettergreepgrense geopenbaar is. afwykings in temporale vloei is onderskei as 'n groep omdat al die foute in hierdie kategorie verband lou met die spoed van bewegings. dit is weer onderskei van afwykings in vlotheid omdat die foute wat in hierdie groep beskryf word, aandui dat die spreker probleme ondervind om die volgende uiting of deel daarvan te inisieer. die vasstekings en worstelgedrag is ouditief duidelik identifiseerbaar. versteurings in die stemhebbendheid van klanke is as klankdistorie geklassifiseer omdat dit ouditief so waargeneem word. dit is waarskynlik die gevolg van temporale wansinchronisasie maar klassifikasie by 'n groep wat dui op temporale versteurings sal impliseer dat die luisteraar 'n oorsaak aan die verskynsel koppel. dit is soms as vervangings geklassifiseer wanneer die luisteraars oortuig was dat dit nie bloot die stemaanvangstyd is wat versteur is nie. selfs in hierdie gevalle kan die oorsaak ook temporale wansinchronisasie wees. dataverwerking: die somtotaal van elke verskillende tipe fout oor vier herhalings van 'n spesifieke eenheid is bepaal vir elke proefpersoon. hierdie data is gebruik om te bepaal wat die frekwensie en tipe foute is wat voorkom by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe en artikulasie-eienskapgroepe vir elke proefpersoon. die totale van die proefpersone is egter ook saamgevoeg (dus die resultate van 20 produksies van elke eenheid en in elke klankstruktuurgroep en artikulasie-eienskapgroep is daar 8 eenhede wat 'n totaal van 160 uitings per klankstruktuurgroep en artikulasie-eienskapgroep lewer) ten einde te bepaal wat die frekwensie en tipe fout is wat voorkom by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe en artikulasie-eienskapgroepe. verder is die verdelingsvrye variansie-analiseprosedure van friedman in 'n horisontale en vertikale rigting toegepas op die data (tabelle 5 en 9) ten einde die betekenisvolheid van verskille in frekwensie en tipe foute te bepaal. die foutkategoriee is ook in dalende volgorde van foutvoorkoms gerangskik by al die klankstruktuurgroepe, artikulasie-eienskapgroepe en proefpersone. verder is 'n ooreenstemmingsanaliseprosedure toegepas om die assosiasiesterkte tussen verskillende foutkategoriee en klankstruktuurgroepe en tussen foutkategoriee en eenhede vas te stel. resultate: die frekwensie van foute by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe: die gegewens in tabel 4 toon aan dat die grootste getal foute die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 a. van der merwe, i.c. uys, j.m. loots, r.j. grimbeek by klankstrukture 3 en 5 (s3 en s5) voorgekom het. klankstrukture 1 en 2 vorm 'n tweede groep wat minder foute veroorsaak en die minste foute kom voor by s4. dit wil dus voorkom asof die getal foute die moeilikheidsgraad van 'n uiting reflekteer. die totaalwaardes in tabel 4 dui dus daarop dat die langste strukture die meeste foute veroorsaak. dit is in 'n sekere sin vanselfsprekend, maar 'n interessante verskynsel is dat daar nie 'n proporsionele toename in getal foute is nie. klankstruktuur 3 is slegs een klank langer as s2, maar vertoon baie meer foute. daar is dus ook ander faktore as bloot die lengte van 'n uiting wat 'n effek het op moeilikheidsgraad. 'n moontlike faktor is die ritme van 'n uiting en ook die spesifieke struktuur wat moontlik moeiliker is om te beplan. by sekere foutkategoriee soos byvoorbeeld distorsie en afwykings in temporale vloei, vertoon s3 meer foute as s5. die spreker was geneig om die kvkv-gedeelte van s3 as 'n eenheid te produseer en die finale konsonant as 'n ge'isoleerde klank wat moontlik afsonderlik beplan word. die besondere struktuur van s3 het dus waarskynlik die moeilikheidsgraad daarvan verhoog. ten einde te bepaal of die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting 'n effek het op die frekwensie van foute binne elke foutkategorie is 'n tweerigting-variansie-analise uitgevoer op die data wat aangegee word in tabel 4. die p-waardes toon aan dat geen betekenisvolle verskille voorkom tussen die getal foute by die verskillende klankstrukture nie. die numeriese waardes in tabel 4 toon egter aan dat daar wel verskille is tussen die getal fcrnte by die verskillende klankstrukture binne elke foutkategorie. klankstruktuur 5 lei byvoorbeeld tot 126 vervangings terwyl s4 net tot 4 lei. hierdie gegewens word ook weerspieel deur die analise waarin die tipe foute wat by die verskillende klankstrukture voorkom, nagegaan word. die tipe foute by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe: die aantal foute in elke foutkategorie by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe word ook vervat in tabel 4. die tipe foute wat by elke klankstruktuurgroep voorkom, kan deur verwerking van hierdie data bepaal word. die foutkategoriee wat die hoogste voorkoms vertoon, is afwykings in temporale vloei (35,6%) en dan distorsie (30,7%). al die ander foutkategoriee vertoon 'n veel kleiner voorkomspersentasie. afwykings in prosodie en veranderinge van struktuur neem die derde plek in beide met 'n persentasie van 9,6%. klankvervangings neem die vierde plek in (8,5%) en daarna volg afwykings in vlotheid (4,7%). foute gevolg deur selfkorreksie (1,3%) het die heel minste voorgekom (kyk tabel 4). ten einde te bepaal of daar betekenisvolle verskille is tussen die voorkoms van die verskillende foutkategoriee by elke klankstruktuurgroep is 'n tweerigting-variansie-analise uitgevoer op die data in tabel 4. die p-waarde van die globale analise toon aan dat daar 'n betekenisvolle verskil (op die 5% peil van betekenis) is tussen die frekwensie van voorkoms van die verskillende foutkategoriee. die verskillende tipe foute kom dus nie in dieselfde mate by hierdie groep verbaal apraktiese sprekers voor nie. betekenisvolle verskille tussen die frekwensie van voorkoms is ook by si, s4 en s5 verkry. die verskille wat verkry is, is tussen die foutkategoriee met die hoogste en laagste voorkoms. in die geval van byvoorbeeld si verskil die voorkoms van foutkategoriee c en d betekenisvol van β en f. die verskille by s2 en s3 is nie so groot dat dit statisties betekenisvol is nie, maar dieselfde verskille kom voor by hierdie klankstruktuurgroepe. tabel 4: die frekwensie (en persentasie) van voorkoms van die foutkategoriee by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe klankstrukfoutkategoriee | klankstruka β c d ε f g 1 tuurgroepe vervangings verandering van struktuur distorsie afwykings in temporale vloei afwykings in vlotheid selfkorreksie afwykings in prosodie j , totale aantal foute si 25 (4,8%) 12 (2,3%) 205 (39,1%) 213 (40,6%) 12 (2,3%) 4 (0,8%) 53 (10,1%) 524 ι (100%); s2 22 (4,7%) 28 (6,0%) 128 (27,3%) 206 (43,9%) 23 (4,9%) 5 (1,1%) 57 (12,1%) 469 (100%) s3 37 (6,1%) 92 (15,2%) 169 (28,0%) 223 (36,9%) 27 (4,5%) 8 (1,3%) 48 (8,0%) 604 (100%) . s4 4 (1,5%) 17 (6,5%) 110 (42,2%) 64 (24,5%) 19 (7,3%) 1 (0,4%) 46 (17,6%) 261 ^ /(100%)' s5 126 (19,4%) 92 (14,2%) 157 (24,2%) 186 (28,6%) 37 (5,7%) 14 (2,2%) 38 (5,7%) 650 (100%) totaal 214 (8,5%) 241 (9,6%) 769 (30,7%) 892 (35,6%) 118 (4,7%) 32 (1,3%) . 242 (9,6%) 2508 (100%) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie 17 die feit dat alle foutkategoriee nie dieselfde frekwensie van voorkoms vertoon nie, laat die moontlikheid dat sekere foutkategoriee 'n hoer voorkoms by sekere klankstruktuurgroepe het. om te bepaal of die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting 'n effek het op die tipe foute wat voorkom, is die data verder verwerk. 'n rangskikking van die verskillende foutkategoriee in dalende volgorde van foutvoorkoms by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe is gedoen en word weergegee in tabel 5. meer fonologies gebaseerde probleme gereken word. hierdie foute neem by die ander strukture slegs die vyfde of sesde plek in. klankvervangings is dus op grond van hierdie resultate nie die mees kenmerkende simptoom van verbale apraksie nie. dit is interessant dat afwykings in vlotheid of inisiering wat as kenmerkend van verbale apraksie beskryf word (kent en rosenbek, 1983) slegs by s4 die derde plek inneem en by die ander strukture die vyfde of sesde plek. die lae voorkoms van afwykings in vlotheid in hierdie studie tabel 5: rangskikking van foutkategoriee in dalende volgorde van foutvoorkoms by die verskillende klankstruktuurgroepe klankstruktuurgroepe rangskikking van foutkategoriee si d (40,6%) c (39,1%) g (10,1%) a (4,8%) β (2,3%) ε (2,3%) f (0,8%) s2 d (43,9%) c (27,3%) g (12,1%) β (6,0%) ε (4,9%) α (4,7%) f (1,1%) s3 d (36,9%) c (28,0%) β (15,2%) g (8,0%) a (6,1%) ε (4,5%) f (1,3%) s4 c (42,2%) d (24,5%) g (17,6%) ε (7,3%) β (6,5%) α (1,5%) f (0,4%) s5 d (28,6%) c (24,2%) a (19,4%) β (14,2%) ε (5,7%) g (5,7%) f (2,2%) die rangskikking van foutkategoriee (tabel 5) toon aan dat die klankstruktuur van 'n uiting slegs deels 'n invloed het op die tipe foute wat voorkom. afwykings in temporale vloei toon die hoogste voorkoms by al die klankstrukture behalwe by s4 waar distorsie die hoogste frekwensie vertoon. distorsie neem die tweede plek in by al die strukture behalwe by s4. by s4 neem afwykings in temporale vloei die tweede plek in. hierdie studie bevestig dus die mees resente mening van navorsers (itoh en sasanuma, 1984; wertz et al. 1984; kent en rosenbek, 1983) dat distorsie tesame met afwykings in temporale beheer die mees kenmerkende simptome van verbale apraksie is. die derde plek by si, s2 en s4 word ingeneem deur afwykings in prosodie, maar by s3 en s5 wat die meeste probleme tot gevolg gehad het en die langste eenhede is, word die derde plek ingeneem deur vervangings en veranderinge van struktuur. laasgenoemde kan moontlik as kan moontlik die gevolg wees van die besondere omstandighede waaronder spraak geproduseer is. 'n voorbeeld van produksie is verskaf voordat die persoon dieselfde uiting moes herhaal. selfinisiering was dus in 'n mate beperk. die resultate van die ooreenstemmingsanalise wat uitgevoer is op die ouditief waarneembare foute wat by die verskillende klankstrukture voorkom, word grafies voorgestel in figure 1 en 2. die afleidings wat op grond van die frekwensie-analise gemaak is, word deur die ooreenstemmingsanalise bevestig. laasgenoemde werp egter ook 'n ander lig op die data. die sterkte van assosiasie of ooreenstemming tussen foute a tot g en die verskillende klankstrukture (kyk figuur 1) en die verskillende eenhede (kyk figuur 2) word afgelei van die afstand tussen die twee punte op die grafiese voorstelling. s3 s5 a s4 s2 d si figuur 1: resultate van die ooreenstemmingsanalise om die sterkte van assosiasie tussen die klankstrukture (s1-s5) en foutkategoriee (a-g) te illustreer die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykgs, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 a. van der merwe, i.c. uys, j.m. loots, r.j. grimbeek 3.8 4.3 4.4 4.1 4 · 5 3.5 4.2 c 4.7 3.3 4.8 5.8 β 3.2 1-2 3.6 ε 2.4 5.7 3.4 3.1 2.2 2.6 g 1-8 1.3 2.1 2.3 d 4.6 1.7 1.1 2.5 1.5 2.7 1.6 1.4 5.4 3.7 2.8 f 5.2 5 3 * 5.1 5.6 a 5.5 figuur 2: resultate van die ooreenstemmingsanalise om die sterkte van assosiasie tussen die eenhede (1.1-5.8) die foutkategoriee (a-g) te illustreer enkele belangrike assosiasies kan afgelei word uit figuur 1. foutkategoriee a en β assosieer die sterkste met s5 en s3. meer klankvervangings en byvoegings en weglatings van klanke wat die klankstruktuur verander, kom dus voor by die langste eenhede. afwykings in inisiering (e) en foute gevolg deur selfkorreksie (f) assosieer ook die sterkste met s3 en s5. dit is werklik insiggewend dat s3 en s5 by alle analises 'n groep vorm. dit is waarskynlik onder meer 'n aanduiding dat die data wat verkry is, betroubaar is. foutkategoriee c (distorsie) en d (afwykings in temporale vloei) assosieer die sterkste met s2 en si. dit is belangrik om daarop te let dat c en d sterker met si, s2 en s3 assosieer as wat a en β met s3 en s5 assosieer. dit impliseer waarskynlik dat distorsie en afwykings in temporale vloei meer kenmerkende probleme van verbale apraksie is en dat indien klankvervangings en struktuurveranderinge by verbale apraksie voorkom, dit meer waarskynlik by langer eenhede sal wees. dieselfde afleidings word ook gemaak op grond van figuur 2. dit is baie duidelik in hierdie visuele voorstelling (kyk figuur 2) dat foutkategoriee a en β en ook in 'n mate ε en f nie nabv aan die verskillende eenhede geplaas is nie. dit toon aan dat dit nie algemene tipe probleme is'riie want dit assosieer slegs sterk met enkele van die eenhede. hierdie eenhede is almal in klankstruktuurgroep 5. foutkategorie β vertoon 'n minder duidelike assosiasie met 'n besondere klankstruktuurgroep, maar dit is veral s3 en s5 eenhede wat die sterkste daarmee assosieer. die enigste uitsondering is eenheid 4.8 wat baie sterk met β assosieer. dit impliseer waarskynlik dat die simptome van verbale apraksie nie volkome voorspelbaar is nie. foutkategoriee c, d en g is naby aan die meeste eenhede gelee en assosieer dus sterk met die meeste (kyk figuur 2). dit bevestig die afleiding wat reeds gemaak is dat distorsie, afwykings in temporale vloei en afwykings in prosodie die mees kenmerkende probleme is. 'n interessante verskynsel wat ook gefllustreer word in figuur 2 is dat distorsie (c) die swakste assosieer met s5. die lengte van die struktuur het dus nie meegebring dat die hoeveelheid distorsie toeneem nie. die getal vervangings (a) het egter wel toegeneem. ι opsommend kom die resultate daarop neer dat die mees kenmerkende simptome van verbale apraksie (soos dit deur hierdie studie aangetoon is) afwykings in temporale vloei, distorsie en afwykings in prosodie is. hierdie simptome vertoon die hoogste voorkoms ongeag die klankstruktuur van die uiting. die frekwensie van voorkoms van ander simptome wat as minder kenmerkend gereken kan word, soos klankvervangings, byvoeging en weglating van klanke wat die klankstruktuur verander, word egter wel be'fnvloed deur die klankstruktuur van die uiting. hierdie foute word die sterkste geassosieer met s5. dit assosieer ook sterk met s3, maar s3 en s5 assosieer sterker met klankdistorsie en afwykings in temporale vloei. laasgenoemde twee probleme is dus die prominentste afwykings ook by s3 en s5. die frekwensie en tipe foute by die verskillende artikulasie • eienskapgroepe: die frekwensie van voorkoms van die verskillende ouditief waarneembare foute by die agt artikulasie-eienskapgroepe is bereken en word weergegee in tabel 6. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die invloed van sekere kontekstuele faktore op die ouditief waarneembare foute by verbale apraksie 19 tabel 6: die frekwensie (en persentasie) van voorkoms van die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe artikulasie_ eienskapgroepe a vervangings β verandering van struktuur c distorsie d afwykings in temporale vloei ε f afwykings selfkorrekin vlotheid sie g afwykings in prosodie totale aantal foute a l 41 ( 12,3%) 32 (9,6%) 90 (27,1%) 120 (36,2%) 10 (3,0%) 4 (1,2%) 35 (10,6%) 332 (100%) a2 32 (9,5%) 48 (14,2%) 124 (36,7%) 89 (26,3%) 14 (4,1%) 6 (1,8%) 25 (7,4%) 338 (100%) a3 20 (7,1%) 13 (4,6%) 92 (32,6%) 108 (38,3%) 18 (6,4%) 2 (0,7%) 29 (10,3%) 282 (100%) a4 24 (7,8%) 22 (7,1%) 102 (33,1%) 111 (36,0%) 22 (7,2%) 5 (1,6%) 22 (7,2%) 308 (100%) a5 30 (9,4%) 21 (6,6%) 85 (26,6%) 127 (39,9%) 11 (3,4%) 0 (0%) 45 (14,1%) 319 (100%) a6 30 (9,6%) 28 (9,0%) 88 (28,2%) 116 (37,2%) 10 (3,2%) 10 (3,2%) 30 (9,6%) 312 (100%) a7 20 (7,1%) 22 (7,8%) 78 (27,8%) 115 (41,0%) 14 (5%) 3 (1,0%) 29 (10,3%) 281 (100%) a8 17 (5,1%) 55 (16,4%) 110 (32,7%) 106 (31,5%) 19 (5,7%) 2 (0,6%) 27 (8,0%) 336 (100%) totaal 214 (8,5%) 241 (9,6%) 769 (30,7%) 892 (35,6%) 118 (4,7%) 32 (1,3%) 242 (9,6%) 2508 (100%) dit blyk uit die totale aantal foute by elke artikulasie-eienskapgroep dat die verskillende groepe tot ongeveer dieselfde getal foute aanleiding gegee het. die variasiewydte strek vanaf 281 tot 338 foute terwyl dit by die klankstruktuurgroepe vanaf 261 tot 650 (kyk table 4) strek. die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe stel dus ongeveer dieselfde eise aan die verbaal apraktiese sprekers. die artikulasie-eienskappe wat vervat is in die materiaal het dus nie 'n effek op die frekwensie van die ouditief waarneembare foute nie. 'n tweerigtingvariansie-analise wat uitgevoer is om bogenoemde afleiding te staaf, nisvolle verskille bestaan toon ook aan dat daar nie beteketussen die frekwensie van foute by die verskillende artikulasie-eienskapgroepe binne elke foutkategorie nie. j tabel 7: rangskikking van foutkategoriee in dalende volgorde van foutvoorkoms by die verskillende artikulasieeienskapgroepe die afleiding kan egter nie gemaak word dat artikulasieeienskappe geen effek het nie. eenhede met dieselfde klankstruktuur maar verskillende artikulasie-eienskappe byvoorbeeld /b"dth ®fup/ en /b-dthufup/ het nie tot dieselfde hoeveelheid en tipe foute gelei nie. eersgenoemde eenheid wat meer variasie in artikulasiebewegings vereis en meer koartikulasie-moontlikhede bied, het tot 'n groter getal foute gelei. om 'n beeld te verkry van die tipe foute wat meer voorkom by spesifieke artikulasie-eienskapgroepe, is die foutkategoriee in dalende volgorde van foutvoorkoms gerangskik (kyk tabel 7). die rangskikking van foutkategoriee toon aan dat of distorsie of afwykings in temporale vloei die eerste en tweede plekke inneem. vervangings, verandering van artikulasieeienskapgroepe rangskikking van foutkategoriee a l d (36,2%) c (27,1%) a (12,3%) g (10,6%) β (9,6%) ε (3,0%) f (1,2%) a2 c (36,7%) d (26,3%) β (14,2%) a (9,5%) g (7,4%) ε (4,1%) f (1,8%) a3 d (38,3%) c (36,6%) g (10,3%) a (7,1%) g (6,4%) β (4,6%) f (0,7%) a4 d (36,0%) c (33,1%) a (7,8%) ε (7,2%) = g (7,2%) β (7,1%) f (1,6%) a5 d (39,9%) c (26,6%) g (14,1%) a (9,4%) ε (6,6%) ε (3,4%) f (0%) a6 d (37,2%) c (28,2%) g (9,6%) = a (9,6%) β (9,0%) ε (3,2%) = f (3,2%) a7 d (41,0%) c (27,8%) g (10,3%) β (7,8%) a (7,1%) ε (5,0%) f (1,0%) a8 c (32,7%) d (31,5%) β (16,4%) g (8,0%) ε (5,7%) α (5,1%) f (0,6%) rw„ afrlhnnnco tuj f (4) (ΐϋ ι) 1 l· μ l· μ v / is the frequency of the maskee. ζ μ and zv are the frequencies of the maskee and the masker on the bark scale, respectively. l'11^ and l'21^ are the amounts by which the maskee values exceed the masking thresholds, for a maskee to the right and to the left of the masker tone, respectively. if the masking threshold is not exceeded by the maskee, the maskee is inaudible. thus, theoretically, the inaudible parts of the spectrum can be removed without a listener being able to perceive the difference. the masking function as depicted above, describes how one tone masks another tone. it seems intuitively obvious that to find the masking thresholds that operate on a specific frequency component, as a result of all the other frequency components, the preceding theory could be expanded to establish the sum of the effects of all the masking tones. if we want to determine the masking effect of each frequency component in the spectrum on every other frequency component, this sum can be derived from equation (4): l = 20 log a„, with au = jv» + 1 0 ^ » (5) this equation calculates a value for the masking threshold. note that the sound pressure amplitudes in pascal/ m 2 are summed, and not the db spl values. this sum is then converted back to db spl. the two summations are used to calculate the contributions to the masking of respectively all the components lower, and all the components higher than the specific maskee frequency under consideration (f). for frequency components higher than the maskee frequency f , masker contributions are calculated by taking into account their masking threshold slopes on their lower frequency sides (sj = -27 db/bark). for frequencies lower than f , s 2 from equation (3) is used. this analysis is adequate for exploratory experiments on the effects of masking in speech. method for a two-tone experiment, masking is easily established in a psychoacoustic experiment (javel, 1981). in order to investigate in a psychoacoustic experiment whether masking does occur in the auditory processing of the complex speech spectrum in the way predicted by equation (5), the test will be whether or not the information theorized to be redundant (the information below the masking threshold calculated from equation (5)), is audible or i n a u d i b l e . as a first e x p l o r a t i o n , a simple psychoacoustic experiment was devised. the equations above (1-5) were implemented in a computer program. the program takes normal speech as input, and outputs a "distorted" version of this speech signal (all information below the masking threshold is regarded as redundant and is discarded). the operation of the program is briefly described. the input signal is a file of prerecorded speech data. the data comes from a calibrated microphone, and as such each value of the data is a digital representation of a voltage. data samples were taken at a frequency of 8 khz. the voltage values can be converted to spl values if the characteristics of the microphone are known. for the conversion the equation used is ν(μν)=100 < μ 7 5 s p u i b > ο·' which was established empirically for the specific microphone used. after the conversion to spl values, the time domain signal is transformed to the frequency domain using the fast fourier transform. the masking threshold in the frequency domain is then calculated according to the equations given earlier (5). the masking threshold is then compared to the spectrum of the original signal, and where the spectrum does not exceed the threshold, the spectral information is discarded. discarding of sections of the spectrum does not mean that we can merely make those values zero, because zeros in the spectrum cause echoes in the resultant sound. a discarding function was therefore implemented, as explained later. a minimum of 10 db was chosen as the minimum value that any spectral component can assume. 10 db was chosen as a minimum, because it is far below the normal 30-40 db ambient noise. after thresholding, the thresholded spectrum is transformed to the time domain by the inverse fourier transform. this data is then output through a digital to analog converter, amplifier and loudspeaker. the quality of speech after masking could only be determined qualitatively because of a lack of quantitative measures of speech quality. mathematical measures, e.g., mean square error, is inadequate for the measurement of speech quality. a reasonable objective measure is described in schroeder (1979), where the masking functions are used to calculate a single value as a measure for quality. for reliable qualitative determination of sound quality, a reference is needed, and this reference is used in paired comparison tests. two references were used. the original signal was used as the one reference, and the other reference was the speech signal thresholded by a level threshold, which was initially set at 25 db spl. 25 db was used as threshold, as with this choice about 50 % of the signal spectrum was below the threshold, which was more or less the same amount of data below the calculated masking threshold for the specific input speech signal. the speech signal, distorted by applying the calculated masking threshold and discarding redundant information, was then compared to these two reference signals. in further experiments the threshold was translated-linearly upward, resulting in more of the original spectrum falling below the threshold and therefore being discarded. the purpose is explained in the discussion. the same linear translation was done with the level threshold, always ensuring that the percentage of discarded data remained similar for the level threshold and the threshold calculated from the masking functions. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) masking property of the auditory system: the masking of speech signals 71 the purpose of this experiment was to establish whether the discarding of supposedly redundant information w a s p e r c e p t i b l e . uninformed listeners were asked to grade the quality of three different speech signals: the original, the signal distorted b y a level threshold, and distortion by a threshold calculated from the masking functions. two implementations of the discarding function were used: (1) the discarded values were set equal to 10 db, (2) the discarded values were taken as value (n) = value (n-1) χ 0.9. this simply gives a gentle decay to 10 db, instead of an abrupt transition. deep holes in the spectrum have the perceptual effect of sounding like echoes. also, normally abrupt transitions carry speech information (e.g., the sharp transitions found in start and stop consonants). thus, the way in which the redundant data are discarded, influences the perceptual quality of the thresholded speech signal, while not having any relation to the effects of masking. ib establish the occurrence of masking in the way predicted by equation (5), we simply need to demonstrate that random alterations can be made to the part of the signal below threshold, without any perceptible difference in the signal. any alteration is fine, on two conditions: (1) no deep holes in the spectrum are allowed and (2) the changed section of the signal must still be below threshold. results examples of the thresholding process and the resultant signal are given in figures 3 and 4. the results of the grading experiments are given in table 1 below. method 1 refers to the method in which discarded values are set equal to 10 db. method 2 refers to the method in which the gentle decay function was implemented. the percentages refer to the amount of data that has been discarded. the discarding of approximately 50 % of the original spectral data occurs for the specific input speech (phonetically balanced sentences) when equation (5) is applied. thus, the 50 % case in the table is without any upward translation of the threshold curve. the numbers in the table refer to the grading given by the listeners, where 1 is the best and 4 the worst. where the same grading is given in two columns, the differences between these two sounds were imperceptible. with 50 % of the signal below threshold, no difference between any of the signals is'discernible. although this might seem amazing, most of the data that were discarded, were at the higher frequencies (figure 4), where the frequency sensitivity is not as high. this means that the periodic time structure of the time domain signal is wellpreserved, and no audible pitch distortion is observed. at 75 % discarded information, the difference between the threshold signals and the original becomes audible, although not considerably. interestingly, the quality of sound from the level threshold was rated the same as the masking threshold. at 90 %, method 2 gave the best thresholded sound quality. the level threshold gave the worst sound quality by far. in both the 75 % and the 90 % case, the method 2 sounded better than method 1. figure 3. the original spectrum before thresholding, plotted from 0 hz to 4000 hz (x-axis). the y-axis gives the spectral amplitude in db spl. the scaling is not shown and is not important. figure 4. the spectrum after thresholding, plotted from 0 hz to 4000 hz (x-axis). the y-axis is the amplitude in db spl. the threshold is the smooth line. the jagged line is the spectrum after thresholding. the part of the spectrum above the threshold is retained. the part below the threshold is the spectrum after application of the discarding function. the effect of the gentle decay discarding function can be seen clearly at the high frequency side of the spectrum. discussion as is evident from the results, masking does seem to occur in the auditory processing of complex (speech) signals in the way predicted by equation (5). for the specific speech signal used in this simple experiment, alterations in the supposedly redundant sections of the spectrum were table 1. results of the grading of the speech signal quality, before and after masking threshold set at: original 1 method 2 method 1 method 2 original level threshold masking threshold masking threshold 50% 1 1 1 1 75% 1 2 3 2 90 % 1 1 4 3 2 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 johan j hanekom inaudible. although it is not the only information-reduction process in the auditory system, masking does play an important information-reduction role. with masking function based distortion, even with 90 % of the original signal discarded, the speech is still easily comprehensible, although the speech quality has decreased. masking eliminates some of the redundancy in the signal. using the original calculated masking threshold (without translation), the information rate is cut by about 50 %, without any audible reduction in sound quality. the purpose of the linear translation of the masking threshold was to explore the possibilities of using the calculated masking threshold for engineering applications. this shifted threshold is artificial and does not have any direct significance in a description of the functioning of masking in the auditory system. the information being discarded is not redundant and audible distortion is expected. distortion is, however, applied in a controlled way, and we are not discarding more important information from some sections of the spectrum than from other sections, as we are doing when a level threshold is applied. engineering applications of the masking thresholds as they are described here, are among others in speech coding. with a preprocessor based on the masking thresholds of the normal ear, one can apply controlled distortion onto a speech signal to reduce the information rate. as explained earlier, the fact that approximately 50 % of the spectrum was discarded with the calculated masking threshold, was used to determine the level for the level threshold. although the difference between the level threshold and the threshold determined by the masking functions is not directly evident in the 50 % experiment, from the sound quality observed in the 75 % and 90 % experiments it is conceivable that the calculated masking functions approximate the masking process in the auditory system. improvements could be made to the model used for masking in this paper, e.g., by basing the model not on psychoacoustic experimental data, but on physiological data. as has been explained, results from two-tone masking experiments have been used to determine the masking functions which were used in these experiments. the two-tone masking functions were expanded in equation (5) for application to more complex spectra. possibly, this expansion is not the most applicable masking model to implement on complex speech spectra, as was done here. however, no measured data on the masking observed in complex spectra are available (although data for tone/ bandlimited noise masking are available). this might account for the somewhat strange result, that the signal distorted by the level threshold sounded almost the same as the signal distorted by masking threshold (in the 75 % case). the discarding function is not based on any measured data. it is not possible to determine from psychoacoustic experiments how the data reduction in masking is implemented into neural firing patterns. from the description in the introduction, it can be guessed that the information is not suppressed, as in the implementation, but simply swamped. that masking operates like a swamping (or saturation) function and not an attenuation function, is motivated by kanis & de boer (1994) and javel (1981). the discarding function was implemented here in order to demonstrate that the frequency components below the threshold are inaudible, and not to try to simulate the normal auditory functioning. actually, we might just as well have distorted the sections of the signal below threshold in any other way to prove that these distortions would be inaudible. when we do this, we have to comply with at least the two rules stated earlier, and a third rule may be implemented with flexibility: the amplitude in the sections of the signal that are to be distorted must stay within the same bounds as the amplitude of the original signal in these regions. conclusion masking plays an important role in the data-reduction mechanism of the peripheral parts of the auditory system. although the experiment described here was meant to be exploratory rather than conclusive, the result indicates that the understanding of the mechanism of masking that led to equation (5), seems to be reasonable. in orderto gain a better understanding of the complexities of auditory processing, it is important that the masking property of the auditory system is not studied in isolation from the other characteristics of auditory processing. on the one hand, the psychoacoustic study of the masking of complex signals should be expanded. on the other hand, more cohesive cochlear models, based on the physiology rather than being heuristic, should be created to assimilate the available data. references allen, j.b. (1985). cochlear modelling. ieee acoustics, speech and signal processing magazine, 2, 1, 3-29. eddington, d.k., dobelle w.h., brackmann, d.e., mladejovsky, m.g. & parkin, j.l. (1978). auditory prostheses research with multiple channel intracochlear stimulation in man. the annals of otology, rhinology & laryngology, s53, 87, 6,1-39. hanekom, j.j. (1990). die ontwikkeling van 'n suid-afrikaanse bioniese-oor. master's dissertation. department of electrical and electronic engineering: university of pretoria. javel, e. (1981). suppression of auditory nerve responses i: temporal analysis, intensity effects and suppression contours. journal of the acoustical society of america, 69, 6, 1735 -1745. | kanis, l-j. & de boer, e. (1994). two-tone suppression in a locally active nonlinear model of the cochlea. journal 'o/the acoustical society of america, 96,4, 2156-2165. i keidel, w.d. (1980). neurophysiological requirements for implanted cochlear prosthesis. audiology, 19, 105-127|. keidel, w.d., kallert, s. & korth, m. (1983). the physiological basis of hearing: a review. new york: thieme-stratton incorporated. neely, s.t. & kim, d.o. (1986). a model for active elements in cochlear biomechanics. journal of the acoustical society of america, 79, 5, 1472-1480. schroeder, m., atal, b.s. & hall, j. l. (1979). optimizing digital speech coders by exploiting masking properties of the human έατ. journal of the acoustical society ofamerica, 66,12,16471652. terhardt, e. (1979). calculatingvirtual pitchfhearing research, 1, 155-182. zwicker, e. & zwicker, u.t. (1991). audio engineering and the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 13 bilingualism: theoretical perspectives of language diversity carlin l. stobbart department of speech and hearing therapy university of durban-westville abstract bilingualism and second language acquisition are discussed with reference to different theoretical perspectives. an integrated definition of bilingualism is provided and concepts underlying second language acquisition are presented. theoretical perspectives according to dodson (1985), skinner (1985) and krashen (1982) are explored. it is concluded that due to the diverse nature ofbilingualism, a single universal theory of second language acquisition does not seem feasible. the need for an increased awareness of the complexity ofbilingualism and second language acquisition, particularly within the multicultural and multilingual south african context, is highlighted. opsomming tweetaligheid en tweede taalverwerwing word met verwysing na verskillende teoretiese benaderings, bespreek. 'n gemtegreerde definisie van tweetaligheid word verskaf en onderliggende konsepte tot tweede taalverwerwing word uitgelig. die teoretiese benaderings van dodson (1985), skinner (1985) en krashen (1982) word bespreek. daar word tot diegevolgtrekkinggekom dat 'n enkele universele teorie vir tweede taalverwerwing nie moontlik is nie. die behoefte aan groter bewustheid van die kompleksiteit van tweetaligheid en tweede taalverwerwing, veral binne die multi-kulturele en multi-talige suid afrikaanse konteks, word beklemtoon. throughout history, humans have learned to use languages other than their native tongue to communicate with members of other language groups and other cultures. all humans can acquire additional languages, but must have the opportunity to use the language for real communicative purposes. the literature on bilingualism is extensive and comes from many diverse disciplines. this results in many different, and often fragmented conceptualisations ofbilingualism. mccollum (1981) has pointed out that the early study ofbilingualism did not consider the bilingual's use of language within the speech community: neither the sociological factors that determine usage, nor the speaker's motivation for using one language over another in a particular social situation. the important relationship between language use and language structure was therefore mostly ignored. recent literature does, however, focus on bilingualism in both linguistic and sociological terms. miller (1984a), for example, views bilingualism as the complex consequence of the interaction of social and individual variables. the study of bilingualism should therefore include an understanding of the cognitive, linguistic and social development of the individual within the community context in which it occurs. this has important implications for the study of bilingualism within the multilingual and multicultural south african context. the present climate of change in south africa has an impact, not only on political find economic levels, but also on a cultural level. part of this change process is an increasing acceptance and recognition of the diversity of populations and ethnic groups within our society. this seems to follow a global trend where there appears to be a significant increase in the awareness of cultural diversity within bilingual and multilingual communities (miller, 1988). the fact that language is an inextricable part of the cultural life of a social group, and interdependent with the habits and values of behaving and sharing among members of a particular group, must be recognised. it can thus be said that a change in attitude toward different cultures, and greater tolerance between individuals of different cultures is likely to become more important in the years to come. consequently, the importance of a sensitivity towards the linguistic and communication differences between cultures is implied. it is contended that within the present south african context, a greater awareness ofbilingualism and second language development is needed amongst speech therapy and audiology professionals. bilingualism and second language learning must, therefore, be understood within the context of both linguistic and communication differences between cultures. terrel and hale (1992) have stated that an understanding of learning styles is also relevant within such a context, as learning styles are mostly culturally determined. speech therapists and audiologists must, therefore, gain insight into the normal processes of second language acquisition within particular cultural contexts. it is against this background that diagnostic and therapeutic implications regarding language acquisition and language delay must be considered. the purpose of the present paper therefore is to investigate the complexities of bilingualism and second language development. different theoretical perspectives will be explored and facilitating factors in bilingualism and second language development will be presented. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 39, 1992 sasha r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 carlin l. stobbart a. bilingualism defined the simultaneous acquisition of two or more languages in language learning environments associated with biand multilingual circumstances can be viewed as an exceptional and complex process. a single definition of the nature of bilingualism can, therefore, not adequately describe the nature of second language acquisition within a particular sociolinguistic context. the complexity of bilingualism lies in the function and purpose it has within a particular bilingual community. for too long there has been an incorrect conceptualisation of bilingualism, which focused on the rules and abstract aspects of the languages rather than on their use in conversation and communication (miller, 1984a). this was also emphasised by mackey (1962, in mccollum, 1981) when he asserted that bilingualism is not a phenomenon of language, but of the message. such a definition of bilingualism clearly suggests the recognition of the relationship between language structure and language use, as well as the complex interaction between social and historical factors surrounding bilingualism within a particular community. bilingual communities and individuals within these communities can not be described in terms of discrete or absolute characteristics. the language usage within a particular community is, furthermore, relative rather than absolute as it is determined not only by an individual's needs, motives, attributions and perceptions (miller, 1984a), but also by a speech community's norms for language use in various domains (mccollum, 1981). the significance of such a conceptualisation of bilingualism is that it describes bilingual behaviour at the level of the individual as well as at the sociocultural level. it is thus clear that the language choice within a particular community is a product of the interaction between individual factors (age, sex, ethnic group and the individual's willingness to comply with the language laws of the community), the setting of communication (the physical and symbolic setting) and the topic of conversation (miller, 1984a). according to dodson (1985), a discussion of bilingualism must also reflect the relationship between the two languages, and the language environment in which learning and communication take place. the relationship between languages is related to broad social circumstances rather than to any formal qualities of the languages involved. within these social circumstances a language has a certain power within the community and a command of that language will be a prerequisite in gaining access to particular resources (miller, 1984a). it must, however, be noted that the relationship between any two languages is not static and may change depending on the frequency and intensity of contacts in either language, and as a result of changes in the environment. miller (1988, p.241) has described the relationship between the languages as "fluid and dynamic, varying from one aspect of the language system to another in accordance with a complex of socio-linguistic determiners". this implies that bilingualism cannot be understood in terms of two separate language entities without considering the effects of the one language upon the other. utterances will therefore carry features of both languages. the sporadic alternation that occurs between the two languages is referred to as code-switching, while constant intermingling of the two languages is referred to as code-mixing. both of these features are typical of all bilingual situations. bilingualism can further be defined with reference to language proficiency and communicative competence. according to williams and snipper (1990), language proficiency refers to an individual's ability to process two languages in each of the following four skills: the ability to understand a message in each of the spoken languages the ability to respond in each language in a manner appropriate to the situation the ability to read and understand a written message in each of the languages the ability to write in each language. kessler (1984) has-defined bilingualism as a social process that develops along a continuum ranging from full proficiency in two languages to a minimal degree of competency in one of the languages. bilingualism thus results from "efforts to communicate, to take part in that interpersonal interactive process defined by the social situation in which it occurs" (kessler, 1984, p.27). this definition highlights the relevance of a communicative perspective in bilingualism. acquiring a second language is, therefore, a complex process of developing communicative competence, and of using two language systems to communicate with individuals of differing languages and cultures. it is proposed therefore that a holistic perspective of bilingualism is needed to accommodate the complex yet dynamic nature of bilingualism. a holistic view will acknowledge the complementary and interdependent nature of the languages spoken. a holistic perspective of bilingualism will, furthermore, recognise the interaction between individual and sociolinguistic factors within a particular community. bilingualism can be understood therefore within a communicative framework by focusing on both language structure and language use. b. concepts underlying second language acquisition prior to a discussion of theoretical perspectives in second language acquisition and the development of bilingualism in children and adults, certain underlying concepts must be clarified, because these concepts are central to the acquisition of a second language. bilingualism must, therefore, be understood with reference to the notions of communicative competence, language proficiency and language environment. according to kessler (1984), this allows the various dimensions of bilingual language development to be placed into perspective. figure 1 presents concepts relevant to second language acquisition. the various dimensions of communicative competence, as well as other factors that may influence communicative competence in second language acquisition are also indicated. 1. communicative competence ' canale and swain (1980, in kessler, 1984) have stated that communicative competence is an essential part of communication, as it includes knowledge about the language as jwell as skills in language use which underlie the actual communication. according to miller (1988), communicative competence refers to the individual's ability to have basic language structures available, and the ability to use, maintain and structure these towards the desired conversation exchanges. it must be noted however, that the various dimensions of communicative competence are not acquired in a universal order. as miller (1988) has indicated, an individual may have good grammatical competence (due to formal instruction), but poor sociolinguistic competence (inability to use linguistic knowledge), while another individual may have good strategic and sociolinguistic competencies but poor grammatical competence. it is, thus, implied that relative dominance in one competence (grammatical, sociolinguistic) will fluctuate over time in one indi-vidual and between individuals of the same speech community. similarly, an individual need not be equally dominant in all competencies. it is thus clear that the various dimensions the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) u a i : theoretical perspective of language diversity 15 figure 1: a schematic presentation of the concepts underlying second language acquisition and factors that affect the development of a second language (adapted from kessler, 1984). of communicative competence together have an impact on second language acquisition. the dimensions identified by kessler (1984) will be discussed separately. 1.1 grammatical competence. thisisalsocalledlinguistic competence and refers to mastery of the linguistic code, that is, to mastery of the formal features of the language. the phonological, syntactic and lexical features of the language must be recognised and the ability to combine these features in pronunciation, word and sentence formation must be acquired. i 1.2 sociolinguistic competence. previously, the role of sociolinguistic competence in second language acquisition has received little recognition. this component refers to the sociocultural rules of language use, and defines the appropriate use of the language according to the social context in which communication occurs. sociolinguistic competence is crucial in the interpretation of utterances for their social meaning. kessler (1984, p.29) has stated that "this is an intricate developmental process for children1 that takes place over time and reflects aspects of normal maturational processes". 1.3 discourse competence. according to kessler (1984) conversation is a form of interaction governed by rules for the introduction and maintenance of topics, opening and closing of utterances and turn-taking conventions. discourse competence, therefore, refers to the ability to successfully communicate messages during conversation. the discourse component utilises grammatical components (the knowledge and use of language structures), sociolinguistic components (the constraints imposed by particular socio-cultural contexts), as well as the rules which govern ongoing conversation. 1.4 strategic competence. this refers to strategies used to overcome breakdown in communication due to imperfect knowledge of rules in one or more dimension of communicative competence (kessler, 1984). two types of strategies can be identified. firstly, communication strategies, which are devices implemented to communicate effectively, and secondly, learning strategies, which are mental processes used to construct the rules of the language. some strategies used in second language (l2) acquisition are similar to those used for first language (li) acquisition. corder (1981, in kessler, 1984) has said that in the early stages of l2 development children may implement specific strategies but these may change as the child grows older and the degree of l2 proficiency increases. summary the above discussion reflects the complexity of communicative competence. moreover, the acquisition of communicative competence must be understood in terms of the interdependence of the above mentioned dimensions. furthermore, the interaction between the two language systems in the acquisition of second language must be acknowledged (kessler, 1984). the complex nature of this interaction is not yet fully understood. communicative competence can however not merely be expressed as interaction between various components. the synergistic nature of the various interacting dimensions of communicative competence must at all times be recognised. a further crucial aspect, not to be overlooked, is the context in which second language acquisition takes place. 2. language environment a discussion of the language environment necessitates a distinction between language acquisition and language learning (krashen, 1982). language acquisition, according to kessler (1984), is a natural, subconscious process that occurs in informal environments. the focus of this process, through which both first and second languages are acquired, is on communication or meaning. language learning, on the other hand, is a conscious process, with a focus on grammatical competence (language form), that occurs in formal learning environments. this is a process available to older children and die suid-afrikaase tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykins, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 carlin l. stobbart adults in developing a second language. according to kessler (1984) language acquisition is central, and through language learning a system to edit language output is developed. the distinction between language acquisition and language learning must, however, be seen in a larger context. skinner (1985a) has indicated that language is a function of learning, but that learning is also a function of language. second language acquisition must, therefore, be viewed as a learning process. this has important implications for the language environment in which a second language is acquired. a further factor related to language environment that has an impact on second language acquisition is the nature of language input. optimal comprehensible input of language is an important characteristic of the language environment. this must, however, be seen with reference to individual factors in the language learning environment, for example, the levels of cognitive development and life experiences (krashen, 1982). an individual factor that has a significant impact on second language acquisition is what krashen (1982) has termed the affective filter. this refers to the relationship between the affective variables (motivation, self confidence and anxiety) and the process of second language acquisition. kessler (1984, p.32) has concluded that "input is the primary causative variable in l2 acquisition and that affective variables either impede or facilitate delivery of input to the brain where language processing occurs". the degree to which the individual is open to input from the environment is dependent upon the amount of comprehensible input that is received and understood, as well as the strength of the individual's affective filter (how the environment is perceived affectively). the acquisition and use of a second language within a particular language environment cannot be fully understood without reference to the mutual influence of the languages upon each other. we can, therefore, not assume that one language is dominant, either because it was acquired first or because it was the language of instruction. such a view would neglect the impact of the different patterns and contexts of bilingual acquisition. each language performs a particular communicative function and thus serves a different purpose in communication. the individual's competence in either language will be closely related to the nature and frequency of access to the situations in which the particular language is used (miller, 1988). bilingual language proficiency can thus not be understood in terms of separate language entities. language proficiency as part of second language acquisition thus needs further clarification. 3. language proficiency a discussion of language proficiency must emphasise the connection between learning and language acquisition. according to skinner (1985a), language proficiency relates directly to learning, and thus to the acquisition of knowledge. an integrated conceptualisation of language proficiency, language acquisition and learning is thus proposed. in this way, various language/learning issues and variables can be examined within a single framework. this has important implications for understanding the successful acquisition of a second language. the connection between language and learning can be explained by referring to the work of piaget, chomsky and vygotsky on language acquisition. chomsky and piaget (in skinner, 1985a), agree that interaction with the environment facilitates the acquisition of knowledge and language. there is, however, a difference in emphasis, as piaget has suggested that learning and language develop from successive experiences with the environment, whereas chomsky has indicated that interaction with the environment activates what the learner brings to the interaction. the essential nature of language and thought as prerequisites to communication thus become an important issue to consider. vygotsky (1962, in skinner, 1985a) has stated that the union of thought and word produces meaning: meaning is thus a continual interaction between these two components. according to vygotsky, the acquisition of grammar and vocabulary is the beginning in the development of meaning, while changes in meanings and the acquisition of new meanings represent learning. it can thus be concluded that knowledge is a function of language and that the acquisition of knowledge, although related to language, will be limited by the language proficiency of the learner (skinner 1985a). language proficiency is, therefore, an integral part of the learning process. this means that an individual's academic performance may be directly related to language proficiency. cummins (1981, in skinner, 1985a) became interested in the academic difficulties experienced by apparently fluent second language speakers. based on the notion that different languages have different functions in different contexts, cummins has identified two kinds of language proficiency: basic communication proficiency and cognitive academic proficiency. basic communication skills entail the use of surface features of the language, and thus basic knowledge and understanding of language structures. academic proficiency, however, includes higher language abilities. this refers to the ability to "think" by using the language, thus manipulating meanings inherent to the language itself. according to skinner (1985a), this conceptualisation can best be explained with reference to bloom's taxonomy of cognitive learning (table 1). table 1: the relationship between cummins' constructs and bloom's taxonomy of cognitive learning (from skinner, 1985, p. 104). cummins' constructs bloom's taxonomy of cognitive learning basic interpersonal communicative skills knowledge comprehension cognitive academic language proficiency application analysis synthesis evaluation i from table 1 it can be seen that academic proficiency in a language can not be separated from critical thinking skills. skinner (1985a) has thus concluded that language can be related directly to learning. cummins (1981, in skinner, 1985a) has suggested that language proficiency be seen as a developmental progression rather than in terms of distinct and separate realms. he thus portrayed language proficiency in terms of two vectors: the ability to identify and communicate meaning in the absence of contextual clues, and the ability to think (determine/communicate meaning) in cognitively demanding situations. table 2 presents the dynamic developmental progression in language proficiency (from quadrant a to quadrant d) as indicated by cummins. quadrant a refers to basic communication proficiency (context-embedded and cognitively undemanding communication), whereas quadrant d refers to cognitive academic proficiency (context-reduced and cognitively demanding communication). a language learner therethe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) b i l i u a i : theretical perspective of language diversity 17 table 2: the developmental progression of language proficiency (adapted from cummins, 1981, in skinner, 1985a). cognitively undemanding cognitively demanding context embedded a β context reduced c d fore progresses from a to d as more meanings become available. this view clearly supports the connection between language and learning. cummins' conceptualisation of language proficiency (1981, in skinner, 1985a) can further be understood with reference to vygotsky's notions on childhood grammar and adult grammar, represented by basic communication skill and cognitive academic language proficiency respectively. the development of language proficiency along a continuum once again becomes apparent. basic communication skill thus refers to the initial command of grammar whereby the child has a broad use of the phonics and syntax of the language. the child is able to converse fluently at seemingly adult level, but lacks abstract, deliberate thinking skills. as the child progresses in concept development, word meanings change which represents development in language proficiency (toward cognitive academic proficiency). a child and an adult may use exactly the same words, although the meanings may be very different for each. meanings of words change as the thoughts embodied in those words change. adult grammar is thus nearer to cognitive academic proficiency as it involves the ability to deal with meanings and relationships at an abstract level (skinner, 1985a). summary the preceding discussion highlights the various dimensions underlying second language acquisition as well as factors that may contribute to second language acquisition. this has important implications for the understanding of bilingual development within a particular context. furthermore, the relevance of analyzing an individual's language system as a whole becomes clear. manyj of the issues regarding language learning addressed by different theories are dependent upon which competencies are examined and under which circumstances. this leads to different views on which factors will facilitate bilingualism. the complexity of second language acquisition thus becomes apparent which clearly prompts a more holistic perspective on second language acquisition. c. second language acquisition: a theoretical perspective kessler (1984) has stated that to become bilingual is a uniquely human phenomenon, yet an extraordinarily complex process. many different theoretical perspectives have therefore been proposed to explain the acquisition of a second language. it must also be stressed that "theory" and "theoretical research" have for too long been the only input in decisions on methodologies and materials for second language teaching. for this reason krashen (1982) has proposed interaction between the various approaches that influence language teaching methodology. theories of second language acquisition and bilingual development are concerned with the same basic issues as theories of first language acquisition, i.e. rate, pattern and processes of language development (genessee, 1988). an additional interest in second language acquisition, however, in contrast to first language acquisition theories, is whether the patterns and processes of language learning are influenced in some way by learning two or more languages simultaneously; or by learning a second language after a first language has been acquired. we can therefore ask whether there are interactions between the two language systems that result in a pattern of language acquisition that is different in comparison with monolingual development. this is further complicated by the possibility that different processes might be involved in successive acquisition of more than one language than those involved in simultaneous acquisition of two languages. a number of different theoretical approaches are evident in the literature, all with differing views regarding the relationship between li and l2. there is, furthermore, no consensus regarding the process and sequence of l2 acquisition, or whether or not a second language is acquired in the same way as the first language. early theories, for example grammar-based approaches, focused on codifying the second language into rules of morphology and syntax to be explained and memorised. oral work was consequently reduced to a minimum. the real experience of the language in natural communication situations was largely ignored (krashen & terrell, 1983). current theories are, however, more tuned to the communicative importance of second language learning. this means that to acquire communicative competence, the key component is the use of language for real communication and that exercises and drills are not necessary. krashen (1982) has pointed out that communicative ability develops rapidly, and that grammatical accuracy increases at a slower rate and after much experience with the language. the following discussion on current theories will reflect three different perspectives on second language acquisition. 1. dodson (1985) dodson (1985) has emphasised that bilinguals, irrespective of age and environment, have a preferred (li) and a second language (l2). the terms "preferred" and "second" language must, according to dodson (1985), always refer to the languages of individuals as the term "dominant language" refers to the language of groups. a distinction can thus be made between an individual's mother tongue (or first language of acquisition) and his/her preferred language. the mother tongue for many people, is not their preferred language. dodson's (1985) theory explains increasing language competence in terms of medium-orientated communication and messageorientated communication. according to dodson (1985), the process whereby the language learner focuses on language or the language learning process, can be called medium-orientated communication. other utterances, in which the message is more important than the medium, have been classified as message-orientated communication. these two forms of communication which can be seen as extremes of a spectrum within which all utterances can occur, are presented in figure 2. a and β (in figure 2) represent all possible utterances that can be made. these two levels of activity are not restricted to speaking and listening and can also be applied to the development of reading and writing. most utterances are predominantly a or β although a mixture of a and β can occur depending on the speech intentions of the speaker. dodson (1985) has suggested that the acquisition of a second language initially die suid-afrika arise tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykinys, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) increasing second language proficiency — figure 2: medium-orientated and message-orientated communication in second language acquisition (dodson, 1985, p. 337). depends largely on medium-orientated communication. as language proficiency increases, the amount of necessary medium-orientated communication decreases. a developmental trend is thus implied. medium-orientated communication is consciously focused on the "mechanics" of communication, while at the message level, communication is more spontaneous, not consciously dealing with the grammatical correctness of a message. at the medium level, the aim is to communicate to the self something about language and the languagelearning activity. the direction of such communication is thus inward. dodson (1985, p.36) has stated that communication at this level is important as it "helps developing bilinguals to separate their two languages, to reduce or eliminate cross language interference and to develop their facility to codeswitch effectively". at the message level the direction of communication is outward. medium-orientated communication thus refers to the sharing of messages with others on different occasions. language acquisition can, therefore, be described as a fluctuating process between language learning and application of what has been learnt. the fluctuation takes place between bilingual preferred/second language medium-orientated communication and monolingual second language messageorientated communication (see figure 2). as second language proficiency increases, the individual relies less on prior medium-orientated confirmation of any message-orientated utterance. it is thus proposed that second language acquisition can be encouraged by allowing the developing bilingual to experience the fluctuation and interdependence of monolingual and bilingual activities in "varying proportions according to the area of experience and the degree of proficiency" (dodson, 1985, p.339). a positive feature of this theory is that dodson (1985) has emphasised the environment in which communication occurs. * he has indicated that unfavourable linguistic treatment may have a handicapping effect on second language acquisition, thus causing a large number of second language utterances not to be made fully at message-orientated level. this aspect may be related to krashen's (1982) proposal of an affective filter. this means that the individual perceives an unfavourable language environment which then has a negative impact on the acquisition of the second language. it is clear that this theory has valuable features that can be implemented in the explanation of sequential acquisition of a second language. fluctuation between these two levels of communication as part of second language acquisition, is an important aspect of this theory. it is, however, possible that the distinction between the preferred and second language status as explained by dodson (1985), is overemphasised. 2. skinner (1985) according to skinner (1985a, p. 106) the ideas of piaget, chomsky, vygotsky and cummins can be merged into "a unified conceptual construct" as a means of directly correlating primary language acquisition, language proficiency and learning. furthermore, this composite construct can be successfully applied to the explanation of second language acquisition. figure 3 demonstrates how skinner (1985a) has proposed that the ideas of piaget, chomsky and vygotsky be merged. skinner (1985a) has noted that the word/age axis (representing grammar and vocabulary) indicates growth in knowledge and language facility. the presentation of growth against "thoughts" and "meanings" indicates vygotsky's proposals on the unity of "word" and "thought" in order to produce "meaning"'. this can further be interpreted by adding cummins' ideas on language proficiency to vygotsky's notions of thought and word unity. it thus becomes clear that as meanings become available, freedom from contextual cues increases. this provides the opportunity to use language for cognitively more demanding purposes. this unified theory represents skinner's (1985a) idea of a single construct that unifies language proficiency, language acquisition and learning. the dynamic growth in language acquisition, and its link to the learning process, can be seen in relation to knowledge. the value of this integrated approach to language acquisition is that it can be applied successfully to the acquisition of a second language. not only is this model based on sound theoretical principles, but it has clear methodological guidelines for the language learning process. as indicated by skinner (1985b) in this model, the language learner is assisted by connecting thoughts with words. it is thus implied that li will be the language of instruction for l2 acquisition, particularly in the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) b i l i u a i : theoretical perspectives of language diversity 19 language proficiency (cummins) meanings a language/learning (vygotsky) contextual freedom thoughts language proficiency gap level of cognitive functioning 4 6 word/age (piaget and chomsky) figure 3: a unified model for language acquisition, language proficiency and learning (skinner, 1985a and 1985b) the early stages. this may also be related to dodson's (1985) view of the fluctuation between medium-(bilingual activity: l2) and message-(monolingual activity: li) orientated communication. the use of contextual clues as a means of conveying meaning thus contributes significantly to the acquisition of meaning in the early stages of l2 learning. furthermore, the value of this approach lies in the fact that it considers cognitive competency as a key factor in determining the level of language proficiency and the ability to think in the language. as stated by skinner (1985b, p.385), "a programme that attends to the transfer of concepts from li to l2 should result in more rapid development of language proficiency". through this model, concept development can thus be maintained at age-appropriate levels. this means that an effective methodology will ensure instructional content and sequence of explanation on the level of li conceptual development (skinner, 1985b). the particular significance of this model is, however, that it pays careful attention to what skinner (1985b, p.386) calls "the language equivalency assumption", i.e. that l2 is acquired in the same way as li. there is thus an awareness of the methodological problems involved vyhen a language learner is forced to function at a conceptual level below his actual equilibrium in li (see figure 3). this results in a cognitive gap, as the learner is unable to unify adult (more advanced) thoughts with l2 words. skinner (1985b, p.5) has argued that "the gap can also be expressed as a language proficiency gap, because of the connections between language proficiency and learning". he has proposed that the cognitive gap and language proficiency gap are related to the learner's anxiety and frustrations in acquiring the second language. such affective learning difficulties are therefore the result of a disorientation imposed by the methodology of language teaching (skinner, 1985b). this has important implications particularly for adults in the process of acquiring a second language. it must, however, be pointed out that this model places almost exclusive emphasis on language proficiency. as discussed previously, this is only one of the significant issues underlying second language acquisition. it is suggested that the use of language (i.e. communicative competence) should be acknowledged more explicitly. in addition, more emphasis is needed ori issues related to factors in the language environment conducive to successful acquisition of a second language. 3. krashen (1982) according to krashen (1982) the best methods for second language teaching are those that supply comprehensible input in low anxiety situations, containing messages that students really want to hear. it is suggested by krashen and terrell (1983) that language acquisition occurs mainly through comprehensible input, viz. by understandable auditory and written input. they have described the following four principles in their theory of second language acquisition: comprehension precedes production, thus emphasising that acquisition is the basis for production ability and that for acquisition to take place, the message must be understood production takes place in stages, meaning that an individual is not forced to speak unless he/she is ready to do so the syllabus should consist of communicative goals where the focus of language activities is organised by topic not by grammatical structure all language acquisition activities must foster a lowering of the affective filter of the acquirer. based on these principles, five hypotheses have been formulated as part of this second language acquisition theory. these hypotheses are not absolute, but subject to change. 3.1 the acquisition-learning hypothesis. according to this hypothesis, there are two distinct ways in which an adult can acquire a second language. firstly through language acquisition, which means that language is used for communicative purposes, and secondly through language learning, thus implying conscious knowledge about the language. this is an important distinction because it determines the nature of language input from the language environment. it is thus implied that language teaching is too often aimed at learning, and not acquisition of the second language. 3.2 the natural order hypothesis. according to this hypothesis, grammatical structures are acquired (not necessarily learned) in a predictable order: certain structures are acquired early and others late, allowing for the possibility die suid-afrikaase tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykins, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 carlin l. stobbart figure 4: average order of acquisition of grammatical morphemes for english as a second language: children and adults (from krashen & terrell, 1983, p. 29). that structures may be acquired in groups. regarding second language acquisition in adults, krashen and terrell (1983) have indicated that a natural order for the acquisition of morphemes exists. this is shown in figure 4. the fact that the natural order for adults acquiring a second language appears under certain conditions, forms the basis of the following hypothesis. 3.3 the monitor hypothesis. this hypothesis emphasises the limited function of conscious learning as a language monitor (editor) in adult second language performance. fluency in production is thus the result of what is acquired in natural communication situations, whereas formal language knowledge has the function of checking and correcting the output of the acquired system. the monitor hypothesis claims that conscious learning has value as a "language monitor" only and that it is not used to initiate production in a second language (krashen, 1982). 3.4 the input hypothesis. input has become a central issue in many theories of second language acquisition. krashen (1982) has emphasised the importance of input over other aspects of second language learning in his argument that output, or opportunities to use the language productively, are not necessary to develop production skills. in the hypothesis, therefore, it is claimed that language acquisition occurs through the understanding of messages, that is, through receiving comprehensible input. efficient input is described as a message that involves general language difficulty not exceeding the learner's ability (dornyei, 1991). krashen and terrell (1983) have argued that when input is enough, the equation of i + 1 will be satisfied, where stage i refers to the acquirer's level of competence and 1 refers to the stage immediately following i along some natural order. this has important implications for the language material the learner is exposed to as part of the acquisition process. the practical significance of this hypothesis is, however, questioned by dornyei (1991) as he has contended that it does not serve as a satisfactory guide for language teachers. furthermore, genessee (1988) has argued that more attention should be given to the role of output. swain (1985, in dornyei, 1991) has argued that comprehensible input is not the most important ingredient in language teaching. an output hypothesis was thus proposed by swain (1985, in dornyei, 1991) to fulfil functions in communication not met by the input hypothesis. efficient output thus involves more than uncontrolled learner talk. the language learner should, according to the output hypothesis, be prompted to express messages more appropriately and more precisely. it thus appears that fluctuation between these hypotheses may result in optimal language learning conditions. 3.5 the affective filter hypothesis. according to this hypothesis that attitudinal and affective variables relate to second language achievement, language learners with optimal attitudes have a lower affective filter, meaning that they are more open to input. according to krashen and terrell (1983) the right attitude will encourage the learner to obtain more input and to interact with speakers of the target language, while at the same time, being receptive to the input they receive. a review of the literature indicates that this hypothesis has the most important impact on second language acquisition theory as many other theories have been altered to accommodate the role of affective variables. it has implications not only for the language environment, which should present favourable language conditions to foster positive attitudes, but also for the individual language learner, to be aware of environmental factors and his/her own response to these. although krashen's (1982) theory of language learning has been the source of much controversy and academic discussion, it can be viewed as a comprehensive theory of second language acquisition, mostly applicable to older language learners. dornyei (1991, p.33) has pointed out that "this theory has undoubtedly succeeded in bridging the gap between linguistic theory and actual language teaching". an advantage of this theory is that it focuses on the acquisition of communicative competence in natural communication settings. in addition, the emphasis in this theory on affective and attitudinal variables makes a valuable contribution to the theoretical perspectives on second language acquisition. from the above discussion on three theoretical approaches, it can be seen that in spite of some overlap in concepts, each theory's main focus is on a different aspect of second language acquisition. the theories discussed are aimed at accommodating the complexity of bilingualism, as well as the various factors that influence second language acquisition. as pointed out by genessee (1988), individual differences im second language acquisition are important issues to consider. factors associated with age, cognitive style, personality, type and amount of exposure, attitudes and motivation have an impact on language learning and language acquisition. the social, cognitive and linguistic consequences of being bilingual must also be considered in a theoretical approach to bilingualism. due to the diverse nature of bilingualism, an integrated perspective on second language acquisition is proposed. this implies a holistic view of the development of bilingualism, based on the interdependence of communicative competence, language proficiency and factors in the language environment! furthermore, such an approach to bilingualism can be based on a composite of the strengths of the various theoretical perspectives, leading to an integrative theoretical view which can be applied to each unique bilingual situation according to the needs of language learners and the features of the language environment. d. second language acquisition in the discussion on second language acquisition which relates to both children and adults an attempt will be made to highlight those factors that will facilitate language acquisition the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) b i l i » i u a u e i n : 'theoretical perspectives of language diversity 21 • children. kessler (1984) has indicated that for an understanding of bilingualism in children one must consider the developmental perspectives of communicative competence for very young children, older pre-school children and schoolaged children. furthermore, the relationships between the bilingual child's two languages, as well as aspects of communicative competence related to school contexts, must all be taken into account. the following two points are important to consider when discussing second language acquisition. simultaneous versus sequential acquisition. kessler (1984) has pointed out that bilingualism in children is related to the timing of acquisition of the two languages. the process of acquiring two languages simultaneously is referred to by swain (1972, in kessler, 1984) as first language bilingualism. according to schiff-myers (1992) this can also be called infant bilingualism. the child develops both languages simultaneously in naturalistic situations. ben-zeev (1984) has stated that bilingualism can also develop in a situation where the child experiences language-switching in the environment from a very young age, as in the case of a caretaker switching from one language to the other whilst talking to the child. this type of developmental bilingualism begins at the onset of language in infancy and refers to the acquisition of two languages prior to age three. the acquisition of another language after this point is referred to as sequential or successive bilingualism. this occurs when one language follows or is second to the first in acquisition order. schiff-myers (1992) has referred to this as childhood bilingualism. the child hears only one language in the home and is only later exposed to a second language. second language acquisition in children must, however, be separated into l2 acquisition in the pre-school years and l2 acquisition in the school years. this is an important distinction as the older child is at a higher maturational level and, during the school years literacy, reading and writing tasks become an integral part of the total process of becoming bilingual. schiff-myers (1992, p.29) has referred to these children as "sequential or consecutive language learners with the first language designated as li and the second language as l2". psycholinguistic similarities and differences between li and l2 acquisition. authors seem to differ regarding the linguistic faculties or linguistic principles involved in first (li) and second language acquisition (l2). schachter (1986, in genessee, 1988) has identified certain basic differences in first and second language acquisition. schachter has cited evidence that older second language learners, in contrast to first language learners, often fail to acquire basic linguistic structures or acquire incorrect or incomplete language forms, thus they do not always acquire complete competence in the target language. the first language, furthermore, has a considerable influence on the acquisition of the second language. this may be due to the nature of the learning environments in which a second language is acquired. according to schachter (1986, in genessee, 1988), these differences do not occur with acquisition of two languages simultaneously during the period of primary language development. the role of cognitive factors in li and l2 acquisition must also be acknowledged. according to genessee (1988) cognitive factors of a non-linguistic nature (i.e. memory capacity and perceptual-motor abilities) influence the child's ability to perceive and to produce particular linguistic structures in first language acquisition. "the determining factors in primary language development are largely cognitive or conceptual in nature because the language acquisition device is a given, while the determining factors in subsequent language learning are largely linguistic in nature since cognitive/perceptual | maturity is a given" (genessee, 1988, p.67). a further factor to consider in li and l2 acquisition is the relationship between the two languages. research has shown that the conditions under which, and the extent to which, a second language learner uses the first language when processing the second language are important factors to consider. genessee (1988) has stated that the older language learner's knowledge base includes specific contextual knowledge regarding social relations within a particular social group. such established conversation knowledge may, therefore, have an effect on the acquisition of a second language. in contrast to the older second language learner, the pre-schooler acquires language in parallel with social knowledge and thus the influence of established social knowledge on language acquisition is not profound. the conclusion that can be drawn from these theoretical arguments is that bilingual development is psycholinguistically distinct from monolingual or first language acquisition. genessee (1988) has summarised such research by stating that children acquiring two languages simultaneously initially go through a stage of extensive linguistic interaction when the two language systems are mixed. this can easily be seen as a stage of linguistic confusion or as a unitary, undifferentiated language system. these points will be discussed further in the following section. 1. simultaneous acquisition of languages in young children two patterns of simultaneous acquisition can be identified. firstly, where the child is presented with different languages in a one-person, one-language association and secondly, where the languages are not person specific and are alternated in the same discourse situation. extensive code-switching may thus, for some children, be the primary type of input rather than one which clearly distinguishes two language systems (kessler, 1984). evidence of uneven development during the simultaneous acquisition of languages must be acknowledged. children often develop faster in the language to which they are most often exposed. the quantity of language input within a particular language environment may, therefore, provide balanced exposure to both languages or greater quantity for one of the languages. these factors interact in complex ways and can, according to kessler (1984), affect the degree of bilingualism, the rate of realisation of two distinct language systems and the degree of negative language transfer (li and l2 interference) between the two languages. according to kessler (1984), such interference, or code-mixing seems to occur when the adult input is characterised by extensive codeswitching. code-mixing refers to "a sophisticated, rule governed communicative device used by linguistically competent bilinguals to achieve a variety of communicative goals" (genessee, 1988, p.69). mixing the linguistic elements from one language into another is constrained so that the syntactic rules of both languages are respected. switching between languages occurs as a function of certain sociolinguistic factors, such as the setting, and tone and purpose of the communication situation. fatini (1976, in kessler, 1984, p.36) has stated that "the more separate the environments in which each language is used and the more consistent the language use within each of these environments, the more rapidly and the more easily bilingual children learn to differentiate their linguistic systems". it can thus be seen that the nature of linguistic input is an important aspect of the language environment as a facilitating factor ofbilingualism in young children. kessler (1984) has highlighted two stages in simultaneous language acquisition. 1.1 undifferentiated single-language system. input taken from both languages is acted upon by cognitive processes die suid-afrikaase tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykins, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 carlin l. stobbart to form a unique language system consisting of elements of both languages. the two languages are, therefore, not encoded separately but as a common core of rules. code-mixing is a typical and natural feature of simultaneous acquisition. there are at least two sources of code-mixing at this level. firstly, the language input may be a mixed code. secondly, during the simultaneous acquisition of two languages, a period of insufficient metalinguistic skill prevents the child from separating the two languages. as a consequence, a mixed language develops along similar lines to a single language. a functional and acquisitional differentiation emerges around the age of two years and six months (miller, 1988). it is then, that codeswitching as opposed to habitual code-mixing gradually becomes predominant. another phenomenon evident in sequential acquisition is a silent period during which the child has limited l2 expressive language, despite good comprehension of the language. this may be a period of consolidating knowledge of l2 to enable the child to use l2 without having to resort to li. pressure on children to communicate during this phase forces them to use li-based strategies to succeed (miller, 1988). 1.2 differentiated language systems. as bilingual children develop they gradually begin to differentiate between the two language systems. according to kessler (1984) the precise age at which this may occur varies as input conditions, language balance and other linguistic and sociolinguistic variables interact. differentiation occurs when the child starts to communicate with different people in different languages. code-switching becomes evident depending on the conditions of the communication situation. bilingual children learn to identify a specific language with a particular person and with particular situations. in order to create conditions favourable to second language acquisition, kessler (1984) has stressed the importance of understanding the complex and fragile nature of bilingualism. should a child be removed from bilingual language input, one language may soon be lost to the child. continued input! however, can stabilise bilingualism, allowing for distinct and separate language use. kessler (1984) has emphasised that for bilingualism to be maintained, the continued use of both languages in communicative, naturalistic settings is required. 2. sequential acquisition of languages this implies that the acquisition of li has taken place before l2 acquisition. it therefore tends to be related to older children and adults. 2.1 sequential acquisition in pre-school children. by the age of three years, children usually have basic communicative competence in their first language. acquisition of a second language at this age thus implies adding a second language to one already in place. in informal settings, pre-school children will direct their attention to the meaning of utterances rather than to linguistic forms. according to krashen (1982) this is an important process used by children for developing a second language. kessler (1984, p.43) has indicated that this process can be greatly impaired "when input is deficient in quality or quantity or when children, because of the complex set of personality characteristics each brings to the task, have negative attitudes or do not have access to the cognitive and social strategies that facilitate language acquisition". this has important implications for the facilitation of a second language at pre-school level. it is recommended that the language environment must be natural, with emphasis on communication. krashen's (1982) emphasis on affective and attitudinal variables in relation to language achievement, provides valuable guidelines in this regard. the aim is therefore, to foster a positive attitude toward l2 in a favourable language environment. corder (1981, in kessler, 1984) has referred to l2 acquisition at the pre-school level as acquisition of an inter-language. this is a unique language system constructed by the child and can be described as "a dynamic, fluid system shifting and changing as the child reorganises it to accommodate new rules" (kessler, 1984, p.43). the inter-language can be seen as a developmental continuum characterised by so-called errors which mark divergence from the native-speaker norms for the target language (language acquired). making of errors thus plays a vital and necessary role for the successful outcome of l2 processes, as they provide evidence of the learner's acquisition strategies. it is possible that dodson's (1985) view on fluctuation between bilingual medium-orientated communication and monolingual message-orientated communication (see figure 2) is applicable in this regard. this would mean that during the early stages of sequential bilingualism, the child relies heavily on the li system, as it is drawn upon for l2 development. as language proficiency increases, li has less influence and the errors observed in the inter-language increasingly resemble the normal, developmental errors which are part of the child's acquisition of the li. interaction between li and l2 does, however, continue, and it must be acknowledged that l2 acquisition is not a linear process. 2.2 sequential acquisition in older school-aged children. increased age, cognitive maturity and extensive language experience are variables which can enhance the language acquisition in older children. according to kessler (1984), other variables such as increased awareness of the separateness of the two language systems, differences in the language environment, differences in language input, as well as awareness of the affective environment in combination with the more highly developed affective filters also influence language acquisition at this level. here, literacy, learning and writing become crucial aspects of l2 acquisition. cummins' conceptualisation of communication proficiency (1981, in skinner, 1985a) has relevance for understanding language acquisition in older children (see table 2). the developmental nature of language acquisition from context-embedded basic interpersonal communication to cognitively demanding tasks in context-reduced settings must be acknowledged. according to cummins (1981, in kessler, 1984), cognitively-academic language is crosslingual and thus applicable to any language context, li or l2. this means that language proficiency on this level, although mastered in li, will carry-over to l2, when sufficient l2 code is available. learning a second language for school use is a task that imposes its own specific demands. the child must become aware of language as a separate structure and learn how to use it in context-reduced forms. this has important implications for l2 reading and writing ability as the child must write or understand a message correctly without recourse to contexts available through face-to-face encounters of spoken language. kessler (1984) has emphasised that differentiation between context-embedded and context-reduced and cognitively demanding and cognitively undemanding tasks outlines a critical distinction between l2 development in the pre-school years and l2 development at school. 2.3 sequential acquisition in adults. second language acquisition in adults can be understood with reference to sequential bilingualism. krashen's (1982) approach to second language acquisition is most suitable for the adult language learner. adults often appear to be more efficient language learners, possibly due to greater cognitive maturity and better metalinguistic skills. affective variables are also crucial in the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) b i l i u a i n : theoretical perspectives of language diversity language acquisition at this level. the language learner must have a positive attitude toward the language and feel confident within the language environment. biological factors, cognitive factors and social factors thus interact in providing optimal conditions for adult language learning. code-mixing and codeswitching have also been observed in adult second language learners. summary the above discussion of second language development indicates that complex interactions of various factors related to the individual language learner, the language environment and the nature of language input determine optimal conditions for language learning. it is proposed that these conditions must be understood within a particular theoretical framework, suitable to unique conditions of a particular bilingual situation. all these factors emphasise that second language acquisition is not a predictable, even process. the process is, according to miller (1988), open to influences from external (environmental, nature of language input, role of first language) and internal (affective, personality, cognitive-linguistic maturity at onset of acquisition) factors. conclusion the aim of this paper was to describe bilingualism and second language development in children and adults. different theoretical perspectives were explored and facilitating factors in the acquisition of a second language were discussed. the discussion highlighted the complexity ofbilingualism and emphasised that an understanding of second language acquisition should focus on the relation between language structures and language use within a particular bilingual community. communicative competence, language proficiency and aspects related to the language environment were emphasised as important underlying concepts to second language acquisition. a holistic approach to bilingualism was proposed. it is clear that speech therapy and audiology professionals in south africa must become(more aware of the implications of working in multilingual and multicultural settings. a great deal of diversity is presentjin the languages used in south africa. bebout and arthur (1992, p.45) have emphasised that "professionals need to become cross-cultural communicators in order to provide adequate services when working with a culturally and linguistically diverse population". this has important implications for the evaluation of a child's language abilities. professionals working within multicultural, multilingual contexts must therefore become knowledgeable concerning the interaction of li and'l2 within bilingual communities. furthermore, the cultural factors relevant to any specific bilingual situation, for example english/afrikaans as opposed to english/zulu, must be recognised. greater understanding of the complexity ofbilingualism within a particular cultural context, and knowledge of the theoretical issues involved in second language acquisition will enable speech therapy and audiology professionals to meet the needs of each unique bilingual situation within the south african context. as indicated by miller (1984a, p. 7), favourable contexts for the acquisition and use of languages will be societies where "pluralism is tolerated, preserved or even encouraged". acknowledgements this paper evolved from an assignment submitted to the university of pretoria as part of a post-graduate study programme. the valuable comments from prof. erna alant (university of pretoria) and mrs glen jager (university of durban-westville) are noted with appreciation. references bebout, l. & arthur, b. (1992). cross-cultural attitudes toward speech disorders. journal of speech and hearing research, 35, 45-52. ben-zeev, s. (1984). bilingualism and cognitive development. in n. miller (ed.), bilingualism and language disability: assessment and remediation. london: chapman and hall. dodson, c.j. (1985). second language acquisition and bilingual development: a theoretical framework. journal of multilingual and multicultural development, 6 (5), 325-345. dornyei, z. (1991, january). krashen's input hypothesis and swain's output hypothesis in practice: designing "i+1" teaching techniques. usis pretoria english teaching office newsletter: english teaching forum, 33-35. genessee, f. (1988). bilingual language development in pre-school children. in d. bishop & k. mogford (eds.), language development in exceptional circumstances. edinburgh: churchill livingston. kessler, c. (1984). language acquisition in bilingual children. in n. miller (ed.), bilingualism and language disability: assessment and remediation. london: chapman and hall. krashen, s.d. (1982). principles and practice in second language acquisition. oxford: pergamon press. krashen, s.d. & terrell, t.d. (1983). the natural approach: language acquisition in the classroom. california: alemay press. mccollum, p.a. (1981). concepts in bilingualism and their relationship to language assessment. in j.e. erickson & d.r. omark (eds.), communication assessment of the bilingual bicultural child. baltimore: university park press. miller, n. (1984a). language use in bilingual communities. in n. miller (ed.), bilingualism and language disability: assessment and remediation. london: chapman and hall. miller, n. (1984b). language problems and bilingual children. in n. miller (ed.), bilingualism and language disability: assessment and remediation. london: chapman and hall. miller, n. (1988). language dominance in bilingual children. in m.j. ball (ed.), theoretical linguistics and disordered language. london: croom helm. schiff-myers, n.b. (1992). considering arrested language development and language loss in the assessment of second language learners. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 23, 28-33. skinner, d. (1985). access to meaning: the anatomy of the language/ learning connection. part i. journal of multilingual and multicultural development, 6 (2), 97-116. skinner, d. (1985). access to meaning: the anatomy of the language/ learning connection. part ii. journal of multilingual and multicultural development, 6 (2), 369-388. terrell, b.y. & hale, j.e. (1992, january). serving a multi-cultural population: different learning styles. american journal of speech language pathology, 5-8. williams, j.d. & snipper, g.c. (1990). literacy and bilingualism. new york: longman. die suid-afrikaase tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykins, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) f" «ν^τ"'5 talking to professionals the needier westdcntr organisation p.o. box 28975 sandringham 2131 telephone (011) 485-1302/3/4/5 mm p l a y m a n d • j s c h o o l r o o m shop 6l the rosebank mews 173 oxford road rosebank jhb. play & s c h o o l r o o m , specialists in the field of child education have been offering assistance to both professionals and parents for nearly thirty years. their expertise and advice range through preschool education, perceptual training, primary and remedial education and adult education. play and schoolroom are sole agents for learning development aids which include an excellent selection of materials of interest to the speech therapist. they also offer an interesting range of aids and books to foster and develop language and communicative skills. their stock of educational books and toys is exceptionally wide. you are invited to view their superb range in their new beautifully laid out showroom phone 788-1304 fax: 880-1341 po box 52137 saxonwold 2132 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 elsie μ. schoeman and david p. fourie being all ears: a systemic perspective on the role of the psychologist in cochlear implantation elsie m. schoeman and david p. fourie university of south africa abstract for those hearing-impaired individuals who do not derive any benefit from hearing amplification, cochlear implantation sometimes provides a useful alternative. recently cochlear implant teams started to incorporate psychological services, mainly as a means to evaluate candidates as to their suitability to receive implants. this paper shows that cochlear implants can have serious and wideranging repercussions for the implantee and/or for the family, sometimes necessitating psychotherapeutic services. a case study is provided to illustrate the need for a much wider role for psychotherapeutic services and to also demonstrate how the adoption of a systems perspective can be of value to the therapeutic process. key words: cochlear implants, hearing loss, systemic perspective, case study, role of the psychologist/psychotherapist. introduction hearing loss is arguably one of the most devastating forms of sensory deprivation (wagenfeld, 1987). not only does hearing loss interfere with the ability to perceive sounds in the environment but, if present from birth to a significant degree, it also prevents the acquisition of speech and more importantly, language, unless treated actively and continuously. while huge strides have been made in the treatment of various forms of hearing loss, there is an unfortunate group of patients who have a profound hearing loss and who derive minimal or no benefit from amplification. research involving cochlear implants originated in an attempt to provide these patients with an alternative sensory device (millar, tong & clark, 1984). cochlear implants have been used to treat both adults and children with profound bilateral sensorineural hearing loss by hearing health professionals for more than a decade. the effectiveness of these devices varies and is dependent on a number of factors. although the implant enables the hearing-impaired person to hear better, it does not cure the hearing impairment. the process of cochlear implantation is a complex one. human qualities like emotions and expectations influence the process and can determine the success of the implant. hence hearing and hearing impairment play a major role in the intrasystemic as well as the intersystemic functioning of a person. advanced technology will no doubt lead to more and more cochlear implants, with younger and younger children as recipients. promotion of optimum development of such children requires that cochlear implants be seen not as a single event but as an intervention that has developmental implications that unfolds over time. the field of cochlear implants is relatively new and research has been conducted mainly in the areas of audiology, speech therapy and surgery. very little work has been done on the psychology of cochlear implantation. cochlear implant teams traditionally incorporated psychological services, but mainly to evaluate prospective implantees for the surgical procedure. it is the aim of this paper to show that cochlear implants can have wide ranging psychological and systemic implications necessitating psychotherapeutic interventions going far beyond such prior evaluation of implant candidates. in doing so, the paper will adopt a systemic perspective to illustrate the role of the psychologist/psychotherapist within the cochlear implant team. the systemic perspective the systemic perspective refers to the cybernetic theory of systems that provides an abstract framework for the observation of human behaviour (simon, stierlin & wynne, 1985). it holds that natural systems or groups or persons, such as an individual, family, or a larger social network, are always part of still larger systems. systems also consist, of smaller systems called subsystems. thus, any system containing an individual or group of individuals is simultaneously a whole unto itself and a part of a larger whole. harvey (1989) illustrates this with the following example: depending on our frame of reference, an individual can be viewed as a complete entity, as the sole object in our perceptual field, or as one part of a family; a family can be viewed as a complete entity, as part of a neighbourhood, or as part of an informal network system. there are thus many levels of organisation in human experience, from the subatomic particle and living cell, to complex organs and organ systems, to whole persons, to families, to communities, to cultures,jand to larger societies. we speak of a hierarchy of biopsychosocial systems to refer to systems that are both a "whole" and a "part". according to bronfenbrenner (1979) these differing system levels appear to be arranged hierarchically, with each level more complex than the one before and encompassing all those that come before it. he has depicted this hierarchy as a set of nested structures, like a set of russian matrioschka dolls, each encompassing the other. from the systemic perspective, an individual and his/her family are viewed as systems within a biopsychosocial hierarchy and the behaviour of these systems is described by tracking the changes that occur within that system and its subsystems, and the system as a larger whole. stated more technically, an individual and family, or family and informal network, are seen as systems of differing logical types within the biopsychosocial hierarchy (keeney, 1983). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) being all ears: a systemic perspective on the role of the psychologist in coclear implantation 17 consequently behaviour of any given system can only be properly understood by tracking changes that occur within that system and its subsystems and by tracking changes between that system and the larger whole. thus, there is a need to "step back" and perceive all systems interacting with each other. the functioning of the larger system cannot be inferred by simply observing each of its members (subsystems) separately (atomism); the whole (gestalt) is qualitatively and "behaviourally" different from the sum of the system's individual elements (harvey, 1989). this wider framework views behaviour as inherently part of reciprocal, circular interplays between environmental conditions and intrapsychic processes. this circular causality (hoffman, 1981) implies that environmental change leads to intrapsychic change that in turn, leads to environmental change. circular causality is in contrast to a linear idea of causation, which focuses on one direction of linear causation event a —• event β —• event c —• circular causation event a f \ event d event β \ y event c figure 1: linear causation and circular causation change; either the environment causing individual change, or individual change causing .environmental change (harvey, 1989). see figure 1 for an illustration of the difference between linear and circular causality, j organisms relate on interand intrasystemic levels and the hearing-impaired are no exception. if a person is diagnosed with a profound hearing loss] or if a person received a cochlear implant, it will not only affect the diagnosed person, but also the family and friends who! form part of the systems of the hearing-impaired person. ; the following is a brief discussion of those intersystemic levels, or nested structures, which are most relevant to the study of hearing-impaired people (harvey, 1989). family the family is the main environment for the developing child, particularly the young child. the family's behavioural patterns, conceptions of hearing loss, emotional responses to the loss, interactions with the child and so on, all exert powerful influences on development. in a reciprocal manner, family development is powerfully influenced by the child and by the demands of raising a deaf child. in this sense, the child influences everything from the use of time and space to financial arrangements, travel patterns, patterns of communication among all family members, and even the family's image of itself— as well or not well, competent or incompetent, and nurturing or not nurturing. in this circular view of causation the hearing-impaired child is seen to influence, and simultaneously and reciprocally be influenced by parents, hearing siblings, grandpareiits, and extended family members. professional as with disabled or chronically ill people, many hearingimpaired clients have extensive and often intense relationships with a number of professional systems, including educational, medical, audiological and other service agencies. for each ramification of hearing loss, there is often a corresponding professional system that can be more or less relevant at the different stages of the deaf client's life. for example, physicians tend to be important early on, with school systems later becoming more influential forces. harvey (1989) pointed out that professional systems and their relationships with family members may become patterned and rigid over time and thereby exert an ongoing influence on the family. for example if parents differ in their attitudes toward plans for their hearing-impaired child, a particular professional's advice can tip the scales. continued support for one parent's position over that of the other can exacerbate a split between the parents. alternately, continued support and guidance toward the child can undermine parental authority, as when a professional exclusively meets with the child, while covertly assuming that he or she does a better job at parenting than the child's actual parents. the undermining of parental authority may also emerge in the relationship between schools, parents, and children when the school and parents compete about being in charge of the child (bodner-johnson, 1986). the interpersonal patterns that emerge between parents and professionals may become so powerful that the boundary between these two systems virtually disappears. therapeutic efforts to help the hearing-impaired child and his or her family are frequently impossible unless the way that professional systems reinforce family patterns, and vice versa, are also addressed. informal informal networks made up of friends and acquaintances of both the child and parents can exert strong influences on family development and thereby on the development of the individual child. the support parents receive may play a role in how well they cope informal networks made up of friends and acquaintances of both the child and parents can exert strong influences on family development and thereby on the development of the individual child. the support parents receive may play a role ni how well they cope with the extra demands a hearing-impaired child may place on them. these networks reinforce functional and family patterns and play an increasingly important role in the development of such individuals, particularly during adolescence, since this is the time the developing child associates with those who demonstrate the identity traits the child longs to emulate. cultural the way a particular culture or subculture views being deaf through its political processes and the manner in which that the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 elsie μ. schoeman and david p. fourie culture provides for such persons, exerts a major influence on the development of each child (higgins, 1980; lane, 1984; sussman, 1976). with reference to deafness, moores (1982) reported "most of deaf people's problems are caused by the dominant society. deaf people have survived and endured in the face of an indifferent world that must be dealt with daily" (p. 141). people who have received cochlear implants, and parents who have children with implants can provide a vital support network for diverse human needs, such as exchange of information and social/emotional support. biological biological factors are important, including the etiology of the hearing loss, the age of onset, the degree of hearing loss, the rate of loss, prognosis for continued hearing loss or gain, the configuration of the audiogram across the speech range, and the amount of residual hearing. there may be related medical conditions in addition to hearing loss, depending on etiologic factors. psychological the particular characteristics of an individual have a great influence on how he or she adapts to being hearing-impaired and on how the hearing loss is treated by his/her family, school and greater society. although such children may well pass through some common and identifiable stages of development, each will do so in a unique manner coherent with his or her personality (palmer, 1970). interaction of the systemic levels all of these system levels comprise the context in which symptomatic behaviour is embedded. it is not enough to say that "it is a problem" or "it is an individual problem" for as was described earlier, a "whole" is simultaneously a "part". consequently, it is necessary to thoroughly understand the interactional patterns within and between each system level in order to provide effective treatment. as bateson (1971) stated, "if you want to understand some phenomenon or appearance, you must consider that phenomenon within the context of all completed circuits which are relevant to it" (p. 244). the systemic perspective or cybernetic theory offers the clinician a more precise map of the interrelationship of symptoms and context. the qualities of systems stability and change cybernetics is based on the complementary relationship between stability and change (keeney, 1983). change is necessary for stability; stability is necessary for change. the theory posits that living systems maintain their form throughout processes of change. this ability of a system to remain stable in the context of change and to change in the context of stability is defined by the concepts known as morphostasis and morphogenesis. morphostasis describes a system's tendency towards stability, a state of dynamic equilibrium. morphogenesis refers to the system-enhancing behaviour that allows for growth, creativity, innovation, and change, all of which are characteristic of functional systems (becvar & becvar, 1996). keeney (1983) illustrated this process by describing a tightrope walker who makes frequent adjustments to achieve balance on the high wire. without these constant yet almost imperceptible changes, the acrobat could not maintain stability on the wire; without this stability, the acrobat could not perform the pattern of changes. the complementary nature of change and stability is also central to evolutionary processes and to the development of new structures in systems. for example, in order to maintain stability, a family must constantly adjust to internal and external changes, such as the developmental changes of its members, diagnosed disabilities of a member, a change of jobs and other environmental changes. a family must constantly adjust to changes within and between the systems' levels of the biopsychosocial field in order to remain stable (harvey, 1989). changes at any level influence the other levels as a partial function of temporal factors. this process is described by the concept of co-evolution. according to bateson (1972), coevolution begins when one system level adapts in reaction to disequilibrium at the same level or at another level. internal and/or external forces may impose the disequilibrium. when a change at one system level affects, and is affected by changes at other levels, the systems are then said to co-evolve with each other. for example, a change in a child's physical symptoms (the biological level) is related to a change in how a child feels (the psychological level), which is related to a change in how the parents relate to each other (the family level), which is related to a change in how the professional helpers relate to the family and to each other (the professional level), and so forth. all of these levels are said to co-evolve with each other (harvey, 1989). recursive cycle within cybernetic theory, the concept of the recursive cycle helps us to track the co-evolutionary relationships occurring among the multiple levels of a complex ecological field (dym, 1987; harvey & dym, 1987, 1988). a recursive cycle is a sequence of interactions that, like keeney's tightrope walker, constantly adjusts to new conditions by processing new information in order to maintain stability. at any given time, the clinician may focus on a specific system level to the temporary exclusion of other levels. for example, at one particular time, the clinician may perceive psychological and communication factors of the hearing loss as more important and thereby perceive other variables as less important, or vice versa (harvey, 1989). the clinician not only continually shifts his or her perception within the ecological field, but also generates hypotheses about the relationships among several variables and system levels, which include the presenting problem. these relationships are conceptualised by the recursive cycle. the systemic model posits that the behavioural and emotional characteristics that may be presented by many hearingimpaired clients have come about, are supported, and are reified as a function of the interaction within and between system levels across time. furthermore, they are context based. the systemic perspective offers the clinician several choices about how, when, and where to intervene in the context of the biopsychosocial field. phenomena at different levels of the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) being all ears: a systemic perspective on the. role of the psychologist in coclear implantation 19 ecology involve biological, psychological, familial, informal network, professional, and cultural influences that co-evolve with each other. therefore, modification at one level will influence, and be influenced by, all other levels to varying degrees. intervention at one particular level may well exert a "ripple effect" across several other levels, and therefore may be the optimal point of intervention. alternately, all the system levels may reinforce each other like glue to preclude effective intervention at any one level, and therefore may require simultaneous or sequential interventions at several levels. the role of the psychologist until recently, not many cochlear implant teams incorporated psychologists/psychotherapists as part of the team. if a psychologist was involved, it was mainly to evaluate and to determine if the cochlear implant candidate was psychologically balanced enough to be able to adjust to the implant and also to exclude any other potential psychopathology. this role is a restricted one. the following case study is presented to illustrate that the role of the psychologist can be more comprehensive. in the case description, pseudonyms are used to protect the family's anonymity. case study: a hearing impaired family the therapist first saw the b. family to evaluate 2-year-old mary for a possible cochlear implant. the mother (lui'sa), father (gregory), brother (sam, 6yrs) and maternal grandmother accompanied mary. the following information was presented: lui'sa contracted rubella while she was pregnant with mary. it was a difficult pregnancy and mary was bom by means of a caesarean section. mary had a weak immune system and easily contracted illnesses. at the age of one month, the family suspected that mary was hearing-impaired. they took her to a paediatrician and at the age of six months a serious sensorineural hearing loss was diagnosed. she received hearing aids at eight months. because of financial reasons, lui'sa had to work full-time. the maternal grandparents decided to move in with the family so that the grandmother could assist with mary's rearing. at this stage mary's brother, sam, was six years of age. the cochlear implant team approved the family and mary, and she subsequently received an implant. prior to the implant there was an article about maiy in'the local newspaper and the family put a lot of energy and time into fundraising to be able to afford the cochlear implant. a year later, lui'sa contacted the therapist and an appointment was scheduled. this time, it was not mary who was seen as the person with a problem, but her brother, sam. lui'sa, gregory, mary, sam and the maternal grandmother were present at this interview. nobody mentioned the grandfather. on inquiry from the therapist, the grandfather was described as a quiet person and not very involved with the rest of the family. lui'sa voiced most of the complaints while the grandmother contributed some of the time. gregory sam's father was, as in the previous interview, quiet and did not contribute much. the family situation was as previously the grandparents were living with the family, lui'sa was in full-time occupation and gregory had begun to work after hours to supplement the family's income. complaints relating to sam included the following: sam experienced problems at school which was in contrast to the previous year, when he seemed to have fared quite well. according to his mother, sam showed signs of disobedience and often cried when he felt that the adults did not want to help him or attend to him. he was willful and told lies. he often verbalised that his parents did not love him. his sister did extremely well with her implant and the grandmother referred to her as "brilliant" and a "star". during the session the two children were playing with toys that the therapist provided in the room. on several occasions mary would take a specific toy from sam. if he complained or tried to retrieve it, his mother or grandmother would scold him and tell him to be good to his sister because of her hearing impairment and therefore her lack of understanding at times. five monthly sessions of family therapy were scheduled. during the first two sessions, the therapist and the family tried to explore and describe the complexity of the problem. sam was the identified patient and there were definite behaviour problems on his side. mary, the cochlear implantee, was the obvious favourite within the family and ignored any efforts to discipline her. there was more than one mother figure as well as an absent father figure. a consistent daily-routine seemed absent in the household. the boundaries within this family were diffuse. involvement of the grandparents, and specifically the grandmother, seemed to complicate the boundaries between the mother and father, as well as between the parents and siblings. the way lui'sa and her mother spoke to one another and talked about gregory instead of with him, suggested a coalition between them. such a coalition might have undermined gregory's authority, which could explain his reserved manner. although the grandfather was absent, the therapist suspected that a similar communication pattern existed between lui'sa, and her mother and father. of further interest, was the particular order in which the members took their seats during the initial sessions. the grandmother and lui'sa sat next to each other. between them and gregory was an open space. the children moved in and out as they were playing on the side of the grandmother and lui'sa. the metaphor of a hearing-impaired family gradually took shape. all the attention and energy were focused on mary, but sam was not "heard". in a symbolic way, sam was shouting for attention by creating all kinds of behaviour problems but nobody in the family seemed to "hear" him. his behaviour gave him some form of attention from the adults in his family and this supplied him with a certain identity. furthermore, the voices of the male authority figures in the family seemed not to be heard either, while the voices of the mother, grandmother and most of all that of mary, could be heard loudly and clearly. mary received constant attention, she received speech therapy, went for "mapping" (the programming of the speech processor) sessions at the cochlear implant clinic, all the friends asked about her well-being and people could even read about her in the local newspaper. in addition, sam possibly sensed that his father's authority was being undermined, which could perhaps account for his challenging behaviour towards authority evident in his disobedience and wilfulness at home and at school. in a picture sam was asked to draw of his family, the father figure was shown as big and prominent with an open the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 elsie μ. schoeman and david p. fourie grandmother i / father mother 1 children figure 2: this map shows the coalition between the grandmother and mother against the father as well as the diffuse boundaries in the family. mouth shouting swear words. this depiction was opposite to the impression the therapist had formed about the father during the sessions. sam could have been expressing the father's underlying aggression or he could have been expressing his own aggression towards the absent father figure. the therapist reframed the problem by composing a structural map of the family. the map is depicted in figure 2. this is a three-generational family and the key issues were: who were the children's parents? was the mother the grandmother's "child", and thus more of a peer and less of a parent to her own children? were they competing for the role of primary parent to the children? was there a parental coalition between grandmother and child to the detriment of the role of the father? by the end of the second session, the therapist decided to intervene in a manner that would perturb the current family structure, to initiate change in the coalitions as described. this interaction involved asking gregory to bring his wife, son and daughter to the next session and to leave the grandmother at home, seeing that she was already working so hard during the week. the therapist reframed it as allowing the grandmother time-out from the family and thereby opening the possibility that she could spend more time with her husband. in doing this, the message implied by the therapist was that gregory was the head of the family and that a change in the definition of his role in the family was required. the assumption made by the therapist was that, should the father's "voice" again be "heard", it could contribute to a realignment of boundaries within the family. in a non-threatening way it also sent a message to the grandmother that her children were able to care for themselves. lui'sa needed support to care for the children and the grandmother it seemed, bestowed the necessary love and warmth. this kind of necessary energy spent by the grandmother in support of the mother was not defined as negative for it did serve a purpose. sam's presenting problems indicated that the time had come for the family to redefine their roles. in the third session, lui'sa reported that there was an improvement in sam's schoolwork but that he still told lies. he also wanted to sleep between his mother and father and he did not want his sister, mary, to attend the family therapy sessions. this was a good example where the change in the family's functioning was followed by stability, in the sense that, although sam's behaviour changed, there were still "hearing" problems in the family. it also showed the symptoms as expressions of ambivalence (fourie, 2003). sam "shouted" but in such a way that he was still not "heard". the therapist requested from the parents their explanation for the behaviour of their son. this enquiry was a way of recognising the parents as the experts in the upbringing of their children. it also relieved the therapist of the expectation to "heal" the family. this process was also congruent with the structural viewpoint (minuchin, 1974) of the need for the parents to form a strong subsystem. in response to the therapist's question, gregory realized that he might not have spent enough time with sam. the opportunity for the therapist to discuss the "hearing impairment" in the family had opened up. it also presented an opportunity to gain clarity on how the parents perceived their different roles. lui'sa responded by expressing the need to be more of a mother figure to the children, not only to nurture, but also to spend more fun time with them. she also said that she knew it would be better if her parents could live in their own place, instead of living with them in the same house and she was aware of the fact that she, her husband and two children did not spend enough time together as a family. she also mentioned that gregory might sometimes feel overwhelmed by the presence of his mother-in-law, and she knew that sometimes she and her mother tended to make decisions without consulting him. thus she indicated an awareness of the need for change concerning their situation, as well as a readiness to realign the boundaries within the family. in responding to this, it was clear that gregory was cautious not to hurt his wife's feelings. he realised that his mother-in-law was a big help to them in their situation but that he also had a need for them to spend time on their own. according to lui'sa they had considered building a separate apartment on their premises for the grandparents. the grandparents could still take care of the children till five o'clock in the afternoon. when lui'sa arrived home from work, the family could then spend time together without the grandparents. lui'sa and gregory continued to discuss the possibilities of being involved in more family activities without1 the grandparents. gregory suggested activities where he could involve sam, for example sam could spend time with him somej of the evenings while he was working. his evening job was 1 fixing cars in his garage at home. the therapist continued to ask j him about his own activities and interests and also about his | perception of sam and of sam's interests. the intent was, once ι again, to confirm gregory's important role as a father figure in ι sam's life and as the person who had the knowledge to give his son what he needed. the parents and the therapist then went on to explore other possibilities of spending time with sam. lui'sa suggested that she sit with sam in the evenings while he took his bath. thereafter she could also read him a bedtime story while gregory spent time with mary. during the conversation, sam stopped playing and came to sit on his father's lap. he expressed his love for cars and motorbikes, just like his father. sam's non-verbal and verbal behaviour seemed to express a need to be closer to his father. the therapist ended the session by commenting in a positive way on the parents' need to be more involved with their children and on the ways they already cared for the family. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) being all ears: a systemic perspective on the role of the psychologist in coclear implantation 21 only sam and gregory attended the fourth session. lui'sa had to work and was unable to attend. gregory said he felt it was an opportunity for him and his son to be together and that had they left mary at home with the grandmother. they were also planning to attend an air show after the session. the change in gregory was remarkable. he talked much more than in any of the other sessions. sam. too, was different. he seemed excited and energetic. he would alternate between playing with the toys and sitting on his father's lap. the therapist asked him to draw a picture of the family (the same task she gave him during the first session). he drew a picture of the family but with his grandmother and grandfather separate in the top part of the house. gregory told the therapist that they had started building an apartment for the in-laws and that they were all very excited about that. he also mentioned that he and sam managed to spend more time together. sam did not display the earlier wilful and disobedient behaviour. he still liked to tell fantasy stories in which he played the main character as if it were the truth. the task of the therapist during this fourth session was mainly to confirm and reinforce the new behaviour patterns between father and son. this confirmation and reinforcement is part of structural family therapy, where praise is given for performing an action in order to help the family members feel confident in themselves and for them to realize that they are capable of doing what needs to be done. the therapist also gave sam and gregory an assignment to work on together in the therapy session. in the follow-up session two months later a strong bond between sam and gregory was evident. gregory was more outspoken than earlier. the family was still in the process of building an apartment for the grandparents. lui'sa, gregory, sam and mary attended this session. gregory announced that the grandparents went to visit their other children in another city. sam seemed to be happy and relaxed and his parents mentioned that they did not experience any of the earlier behaviour problems with him. the boundaries and the communication in the family, as illustrated in figure 3, seemed to be clear and an effective hierarchical structure seemed to be in place. figure 3: a structural map of the family after therapy. mother (m), father (f), grandmother (gm), grandfather (gf) and grandchildren (gchildren). all individuals are equally respected and clear boundaries exist in the family. discussion of the therapy during the first two sessions it was clear that the diagnosis of deafness in the daughter affected the whole family. the parents went to extremes to try to restore her hearing through a cochlear implant. possible guilt feelings manifested in the way they handled the other sibling, sam. he had to accommodate the hearing impairment in his sister at the cost of his own needs. the grandmother involved mary in all kinds of therapies in order for her to be "normal". lui'sa left more and more of her responsibilities to the grandmother. the family showed a tendency to over-protect the hearingimpaired child at the cost of the other sibling. the other members of the cochlear implant team perceived the implant as a huge success, as mary was doing so well with the implant. they focused on the hearing and the performance of the little girl according to measurable standards. this improved hearing was the positive side on the other hand, the cochlear implant and all the aspects surrounding it, contributed to changes in the family's overall situation. the presence of the grandparents and the nature of communication in the family complemented the specific situation in the family where the voice of mary was heard but not the voice of sam. the above is an example of how the change in one member of the family, through a hearing impairment or a cochlear implant, had an effect on the rest of the family system. in this case, the behaviour of sam could only be fully understood when the therapist "stepped back" and perceived all the subsystems interacting with each other. the circular interplay was clear: the diagnosis of hearing impairment and the cochlear implant in the one sibling affected the family, which impacted the behaviour of sam, which in turn altered the family's behaviour. the intervention of other professionals, such as the speech therapists and audiologists, affected the family system and contributed to sam's feelings of being ostracised. even the informal networks, such as the friends and acquaintances, reinforced the focus of attention on the implanted child, thus in a way excluding the other sibling. conclusion it is clear from this case description that cochlear implants can have a much wider impact than merely improving the hearing and the quality of life of the implantee. occasionally, unforeseen and sometimes negative intraand/or intersystemic effects can and do come to the fore. a successful surgical implant and subsequent audiological adaptation do not necessarily constitute the end of the involvement of the cochlear implant team. confining the role of the psychologist/ psychotherapist, as part of this team, to the prior evaluation of prospective implantees is therefore short sighted. assessment and intervention can and should proceed throughout the period of post-implant rehabilitation. while we do not suggest a fixed way of working with cochlear implantees and their families, it is increasingly clear that such assessment and intervention should encompass as wide a perspective as possible. a systemic approach was followed and proved favourable. μ f gm gf children gchildren the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 elsie μ. schoeman and david p. fourie references bateson, g. (1971). a systems approach. family therapy: critical evaluation. international journal of psychiatry, 9, 242-244. bateson, g. (1972). steps to an ecology of mind. new york: ballantine. becvar, d.s., & becvar, rj. (1996). family therapy: a systemic integration. massachusetts: allyn and bacon. bodner-johnsen, b. (1986). the family in perspective. in d. m. luterman (ed.), deafness in perspective (pp. 225239). san diego, ca: college-hill press. bronfenbrenner, u. (1979). the ecology of human development. cambridge, ma: harvard university press. dym, b. (1987). the cybernetics of physical illness. family process, 26, 9-21. fourie, d.p. (2003). limited options: symptoms as expressions of ambivalence. american journal of family therapy, 31, 51-59. harvey, m.a. (1989). psychotherapy with deaf and hard-ofhearing persons: a systemic model. new jersey: lawrence erlbaum associates, inc. harvey, ma., & dym, b. (1987). an ecological view of deafness. family systems medicine, 5, 52-64. harvey, m.a., & dym, b. (1988). an ecological perspective on deafness. journal of rehabilitation of the deaf, 21(3), 12-20. higgins, p.c. (1980). outsiders in a hearing world: a sociology of deafness. beverly hills, ca: sage publications. hoffman, l. (1981). foundations of family therapy. new york: basic books. keeney, b.p. (1983). aesthetics of change. new york: guilford. lane, h. (1984). is there a "psychology of the deaf'? paper presented at the office of special education programs. conference of research project directors, boston, ma. millar, j.b., tong, y.c., & clark, g.m. (1984). speech processing for cochlear implant prosthesis. journal of speech and hearing research, 27, 280296. minuchin, s. (1974). families and family therapy. cambridge, ma: harvard university press. moores, d.f. (1982). educating the deaf: psychology, principles and practices (2nd ed.). boston, ma: houghton mifflin co. palmer, j.o. (1970). the psychological assessment of children. new york: wiley. simon, t.b., stierlin, h., & wynne, l.c. (1985). the language of family therapy: a systemic vocabulary and sourcebook new york: family process press. sussman, a.e. (1976). attitudes towards deafness: psychology's role past, present and potential. in f.b. crammette & a.b. crammette (eds.), vii world congress of the world federation of the deaf (pp. 154168). washington, dc: national association of the deaf. wagenfeld, d.j. (1987). cochlear implants. south african medical journal, 72, 452-454. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 51, 2004 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) biofeedback a n d stuttering dr. e. gordon ( m b b c h . bsc. h o n s ) baragwanath hospital johannesburg dr. a. gordon, ( m b b c h . ) l. gordon, b . a . ( s p & η τ η ) witwatersrand μ. shapiro, b . a . ( s p & η τ η ) witwatersrand μ. mentis, b . a . ( s p & η τ η ) witwatersrand μ. suchet, bsc ( h o n s . ) witwatersrand summary electromyographic biofeedback was used to reduce tension and enhance control of the speech associated muscles, resulting in a reduction of the frequency of stuttering. five sessions were conducted over a course of three weeks. a mild, moderate and severe stutterer were assessed. a decrease in stuttering frequency was seen in each subject from pre to post biofeedback sessions on both a descriptive and an inferential level. opsomming elektromiografiese bioterugvoering is gebruik om spanning te verminder en kontrole van die spraakspiere te bevorder. drie proefpersone met verskillende grade van hakkelsimptome is geevalueer. 'n vermindering van hakkelfrekwensie is in elke proefpersoon, van pretot post-bioterugvoeringsessies op twee verskillende vlakke waargeneem. biofeedback therapy is the technique of monitoring, feeding back and thereby learning control of involuntary physiological responses, or learning more sophisticated control over voluntary responses. a simple diagramatic representation of a biofeedback loop is delineated below. any physiological parameter e.g. the tone of the speech related muscles, are monitored via the biofeedback machine. the response is amplified and converted into a visual and/or audial display, which is easily interpreted by the subject. the subject gains an impression, from the display, of e.g. the tone of speech muscles. it is via this feedback that the subject may learn to re-educate these muscles. the new physiological response is learnt through operant conditioning. a u d i t o r y o r v i s u a l d i s p l a y s u b j e c t e . g . m u s c l e t e n s i o n m a c h i n e die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 106 ε. gordon et al the reward is the sense of success which is able to be interpreted via the biofeedback machine.6' 7 biofeedback therapy has a wide range of applications including e e g , cardiovascular, neuromuscular and gastro-intestinal behaviour modifications. stuttering is considered by numerous researchers to be accompanied by spasm of the laryngeal muscles (van r i p e r , 1 2 ) . a pilot study by richmond et al (1975) reported that auditory biofeedback of laryngeal muscle tension reduced stuttering in a single subject. the study demonstrated that there was an intimate relationship between laryngeal tension arid stuttering. electromyographic investigations by freeman and ushijima (1975) lend support to the hypothesis that the laryngeal behaviour associated with certain of the stutterings, is related to simultaneous contraction of antagonistic laryngeal muscles. since speech is but part of the total mechanism of communication it is evident that the central co-ordination of speech movements will be involved with a variety of additional muscular activities associated with expression (e.g. facial, head, arm, hand movements) and that these may consequently be affected by factors causing disharmony in speech itself (dewar et al 3 ). using electromyographic recorders on the masseter muscles it has been demonstrated that there is an increase in muscle tension in stutterers as opposed to non-stutterers (williams,1 1; shrum, 9 , 1 0 ) . s h r u m 1 0 measured surface electrical activity of facial neck and chest muscles in stutterers. he observed a relationship between increases in electromyographic signal amplitude prior to speech and stuttering. stuttering was apparently preceded by an early and sustained rise in signal amplitude in almost all muscles studied. elimination of covert pre-utterance activity was achieved by employing an audible analog of electromyographic activity to train stutterers to begin speaking with nearly resting levels of muscle action potentials. if feedback is progressively reduced in learning situations, control may become a "proprioceptive" skill permitting unobtrusive and perhaps automatic maintenance of fluency (hefferline,5). the technical aspects of muscular action and of electromyography in speech research have been comprehensively dealt with in the literature (fromkin and ladefoged,4 ohrian et a l ., 8 buchthal2). / / m e t h o d o l o g y a i m to demonstrate that using electromyographic (emg) biofeedback, subjects can control the muscles related to speech and reduce the frequency of stuttering. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) biofeedback and stuttering 107 s u b j e c t s the subjects were volunteers in response to an advert in the university newspaper. the subjects were adults ranging in age from 27 to 37 years and had a long history of stuttering with onset in early childhood. all subjects had previously received therapy at various stages in their lives. no subject had a prior knowledge of biofeedback. there was one female and two male subjects. subject a's stuttering pattern was characterized by infrequent cessation of vocal activity on the initial sound of the word. he was classified both by frequency and severity as a mild stutterer. subject b's stuttering pattern was characterized by sound and word repetitions and prolongations with a rise in pitch. she was classified as a moderate stutterer in both frequency and severity. subject c's stuttering pattern was predominantly characterized by deep inhalation associated with facial contortion, before initiating words. other stuttering symptoms were facial tremor, grimacing, flaring of nostrils and peripheral tension. subject c was classified as a severe stutterer in terms of frequency and severity. m a t e r i a l s a n d m e t h o d two model atlas 251a six function physiological data monitoring and feedback systems were used. for the purpose of this study the emg biofeedback was utilized. signals from two electrodes were collected, averaged and then converted into two types of feedback, a visual meter reading (an amp. meter) and a variable frequency audio tone. a reduction in the subject's tension produced lower meter readings and lower auditory frequencies. auditory feedback was received by the subject through an earphone. the muscle groups selected were: (a) peri-oral muscle groups. on these mimetic muscles the electrodes were placed onto the angle of the mouth. (b) the laryngeal muscle group. two surface emg electrodes were attached one on either side of the neck midline, approximately one cm superior to the thyroid cartilage, hereby assessing the laryngeal muscle group. a ground electrode was attached five cm below the elbow on the ulna bone. all three subjects read the same passage from a child's reading book. they were then asked to speak spontaneously. it is customary when estimating the frequency of stuttering moments to use the above two methods (aron 1 ). in addition the subjects described thematic apperception tests (tat). this was included in an attempt to measure the effect of biofeedback on stuttering while the subject was pre-occupied with creative thinking. all speech was tape recorded so that it could be transcribed and the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 108 ε. gordon et al moments marked and studied by four testers. ratings of all items were based on frequency and not severity of stuttering. the counting of the stuttering moments constituted the basic measurements of the disorder. this was in accordance with sherman et al (1958), who compared three measures of stuttering severity, namely reading time, frequency moments of stuttering and scale values from listeners ratings. interrelationships were found to be statistically significant and the strength of the relationship was highest between the frequency counts and the rated severity. five sessions were conducted over a course of three weeks with equal intervals inbetween. the same clinical therapy rooms were utilized throughout. each biofeedback session consisted of one hour, subdivided in the following way: (i) a baseline stuttering frequency was assessed at the start and end of each session. the biofeedback equipment was disconnected during the baseline assessments in order to minimize the effect of distraction. (ii) in the initial 10 minutes of the first session the subject received a succinct explanation of the anatomy and physiology of the speech mechanism. the potential role of biofeedback in these muscle groups associated with stuttering was explained in an appropriate fashion. (iii) the aim of the first half of the biofeedback session was for the subject to master and reinforce the relaxation of the specific muscle group selected. (iv) the second half of the biofeedback session was to extend this relaxation training to the speaking situation. two therapists conducted each training session, alternating on a rotatory basis to minimize the effect of subject familiarity. results figures 1, 2 and 3 delineate the results obtained. the mean combined stuttering frequencies of reading, tat, and free speech, before and after biofeedback training, are seen in figure one. on a descriptive level a reduced stuttering frequency can be seen for each subject, from pre to post biofeedback training. subject a showed a decrease of 38,5%, subject β 35,6%, and subject c 26;2%. the mean decrease in stuttering frequency for the total samplcwas 33,4%. on an inferential level these decreases are statistically significant. in both the one way and two way analysis of variance a significant difference was found between pre and post biofeedback. this was significant at the 6,1% level. no significant difference was found at the 10% level between the different methods of analysis, nor between the sessions. ; figure 2 illustrates the cumulative mean results of each subject's stuttering frequency, measured at the beginning and end of each the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) biofeedback and stuttering 109 25 η 20 10 5 • before biofeedback after biofeedback β subject figure 1: the mean combined reading, tat, and free speech stuttering frequencies before and after biofeedback. a = b e f o r e b i o f e e d b a c k b = a f t e r b i o f e e d b a c k i l i a i l l b s e s s i o n n u m b e r figure 2: the cumulative mean frequency of stuttering/100 words, measured at the beginning and end of each session, over five sessions. session. on a descriptive level, the stuttering frequency carry over from the end of one session to the beginning of the next is intermittently reduced. however, on an inferential level, this carry over is not statistically significant. figure 3 illustrates the differences in the three methods of analysis of stuttering frequency (free speech, tat and reading). in all cases reading showed a marked decrease in relation to tat and free speech. this is especially noticeable in subjects β and c in which decreases were 73% and 78% respectively. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 110 ε. gordon et al pre post a β c 36 i f 4 > change ii iii a β c 40 73 78 a β c 46 3 2 ι 1 6 . 8 % 0 '7.0,2 t ii β subjects 21.c i (i) thematic apperception test (tat) (ii) reading (iii) free speech (figure 3 demonstrates the numerical and percentage change in mean frequency of stuttering/100 words from pre biofeedback programme to post biofeedback programme;) figure 3: a comparison of the three different methods of analysing stuttering frequency (free speech, tat, and reading). discussion pertinent to this report as a pilot study are additional subjective (patient and therapist) perceptions. most evident was subject c, in whom quantitative analysis yielded the least decrease in stuttering but in whom the stuttering pattern was markedly changed. the subject spoke in a more controlled manner and demonstrated a reduction of head shaking, deep inhalation and facial contortion. this would concur with the findings of dewar et al 3 . he noted that abnormal activity of the orbicularis oculi muscles concomitant with stammering was abolished at the same time as the speech disfluency, by larynx triggered auditory feedback masking. subject a, although demonstrating mild symptomatology, stated that he had benefitted from the programme and that his family had noticed an improvement in his speech. / subject b, felt that he had benefitted in that the relationship between tension and stuttering had been highlighted for him. he felt he was able to gain control of both general and focal tension areas. a positive influence was the encouragement he had received from his family, who expressed having noticed improvement in his speech. all three subjects strongly felt that maximum benefit would be derived from a more intensive programme. stuttering has been reported to be the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) biofeedback and stuttering 111 under stimulus control. when trying to alter a well learned habit pattern numerous opportunities may be required in the re-education, reinforcement and internalization of the new task. regardless of severity, all 3 subjects benefitted from biofeedback training. much criticism has been levelled at current trends in stuttering therapy and it has been expressed that the success achieved in some instances is often short lived and may be attributed to distraction. dewar et al 3 used an apparatus providing an auditory feedback masking noise triggered by means of a throat microphone switch. evaluation tests of this device made on 53 stammerers demonstrated its effectiveness in reducing stammering and abolishing gross concomitant movements. an attempt was made to eliminate the influence of distraction and no biofeedback machinery was used during the evaluation of the pre or post experimental baselines. it was felt that the age of the subjects was a limitation of the study. the subjects had been exposed to many forms of stuttering therapy which have determined their present attitudes with regard to the success of stuttering treatment. an intense training period with a group of adolescent stutterers is being investigated by the authors. with conservative modalities of therapy adolescents have proved to be difficult to work with and often resistant to becoming involved in a therapeutic relationship. the use of sophisticated machinery may stimulate an interest and motivation in the adolescent stutterer. conclusions 1. biofeedback was found to significantly decrease stuttering within sessions in all of the subjects tested. 2. on a descriptive level an intermittent carry-over of reduced stuttering between sessions was evident. on an inferential level however this trend was not statistically significant. 3. a mild, moderate and severe stutterer achieved significant reductions in stuttering frequency with biofeedback training. 4. biofeedback re-education may have a place in the management of stuttering. ackno wledgements the authors wish to express their thanks to prof. m. l. aron, head, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, for granting permission for the subjects to be used in this study. references 1. aron m. l. (1967) the relationships between measurements of stuttering behaviour. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 112 ε. gordon et al 2. buchthal, f. (1960) the general concept of the motor unit. res. publ. ass. nerv. ment. dis. 38, 1. 3. dewar, α . , dewar, a . d . , anthony, j. f. k. (1976) the effect of auditory feedback masking on concomitant movements of stammering. british journal of disorders of communication. 4. fromkin, victoria and ladefoged, p. (1966) electromyography in speech research. phonetica, 15, 219. 5. hefferline, r. (1958) the role of proprioception in the control of behaviour. trans. n:y. acad. sci., 20, 739. 6. kinsman, r. a. g. staudenmayer, h. (1978) baseline levels in muscle relaxation training. biofeedback and self-regulation 3 87-194. 7. lanyon, r. i. et al (1976) modification of stuttering through emg biofeedback, a preliminary study. behaviour therapy 7, 96-103 academic press new york. 8. ohrian, s. et al (1965) electromyographic studies of facial muscles during speech. stl-qpsr-3/1965. speech transmission laboratory, royal institute of technology, stockholm. 9. shrum, w. f. (1967-1968). the study of the study of the speaking behaviour of stutterers and non-stutterers by means of multichannel electromygraphy. dissertation abstracts a. cushing-malloy, inc., ann arbor michigan, u.s.a. 10. shrum, w. (1969) a study of speaking behaviour of stutterers and non-stutterers by means of multichannel electromyography. doctoral dissertation, univ. of iowa. 11. williams, d. e. (1955) masseter muscle action potentials in stuttered and non-stuttered speech. journal of speech and hearing disorders 20, 242-261. 12. van riper, c. (1971) the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs n.j. prentice hall. i the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) does the child hear? in many cases, only the abr can tell infants, y o u n g c h i l d r e n , retarded individuals, a n d other n o n c o m m u n i c a t i v e patients can't accurately describe t h e i r h e a r i n g , t h e auditory b r a i n s t e m r e s p o n s e (abr) is a sensitive tool for objectively identifying and d i a g n o s i n g h e a r i n g disorders in such p e r s o n s . it provides exact information a b o u t t h e functional status of t h e cochlea a n d b r a i n s t e m pathways that c a n n o t b e obtained by o t h e r tests, behavioral o r physiologic. it is a reliable m e t h o d for m e a s u r i n g h e a r i n g threshold. further; t h e t e c h n i q u e d i s t i n g u i s h e s recruiting from non-recruiting losses, and r e t r o c h o c h l e a r from peripheral d i s o r d e r s . p e d i a t r i c e v a l u a t i o n the abr is especially valuable in s c r e e n i n g high-risk infants, testing infants with c o n g e n i t a l malformations, a n d in t h e abr c a n b e r a p i d l y a n d c o n v e n i e n d y m e a s u r e d i n t h e c l i n i c -or at t h e b e d s i d e . c l i c k s t i m u l i d e l i v e r e d t h r o u g h e a r p h o n e s e l i c i t t h e abr, w h i c h is m e a s u r e d f r o m e l e c t r o d e s p l a c e d o n t h e p a t i e n t ' s h e a d . assessing o t h e r hard-to-test c h i l d r e n . such early d e t e r m i n a t i o n s allow p r o p e r m a n a g e m e n t , e n s u r i n g n o r m a l linguistic skill d e v e l o p m e n t . a t y p i c a l s e r i e s o f abrs m e a s u r e d at v a r y i n g c l i c k i n t e n s i t i e s . each abr is t h e s u m m a t i o n o f r e s p o n s e s f r o m 2 0 0 0 c l i c k s t i m u l i . t h e i n d i v i d u a l w a v e s r e p r e s e n t t h e s o u n d i n d u c e d n e u r a l a c t i v i t y a s it a s c e n d s t h e a u d i t o r y p a t h w a y . t h e w e a k e s t c l i c k that p r o d u c e s a w a v e v r e s p o n s e e s t a b l i s h e s t h e h e a r i n g t h r e s h o l d . a c o u s t i c t u m o r d e t e c t i o n with p r o p e r i n t e r p r e t a t i o n , t h e abr is an efficient test in t h e early d i a g n o s i s of acoustic t u m o r s . in short, t h e abr t e c h n i q u e can s e r v e as a significant and v a l u a b l e tool for t h e otologist a n d audiologist. a c o m p l e t e s y s t e m from n i c o l e t , t h e e s t a b l i s h e d l e a d e r i n a b r i n s t r u m e n t a t i o n . the nicolet ca-1000 is a complete, clinically-proven system for recording eiokedpotentials. its e a s e of o p e r a t i o n a n d portability m a k e it ideal for clinical use. nicolet's 14 years of l e a d e r s h i p in e v o k e d potential t e c h n o l o g y p r o v i d e t h e ca-1000 u s e r with an u n e q u a l l e d s u p p o r t system, including r e g i o n a l sales and service, o n s i t e training, a n d w o r k s h o p s o n r e c o r d i n g t e c h n i q u e s a n d diagnostic applications. please phone or write for further details or to arrange for a demonstration. -8 conductive sensorineural ο y v hearing . recruiting = / \ ioss λ loss high 2 v a frequency / n v v. λ, loss υ,1 normal range n y / ^ > > stimulus intensity (decibels) c u r v e s o f w a v e v l a t e n c i e s a s a f u n c t i o n o f i n t e n s i t y . c u r v e s o b t a i n e d f r o m h e a r i n g i m p a i r e d p a t i e n t s differ f r o m t h e n o r m a l c u r v e . t h e s e d i f f e r e n c e s d e t e r m i n e b o t h m a g n i t u d e a n d t y p e o f h e a r i n g l o s s . galambos r, hero* kj clinical applications of the auditory brainstem response. otolaryngol. clin. north am. 11:709-722,1978. d e f e r e n c e · plcton τ w, et. al: evoked potential audiometry. j. otolaryng. 6:90-119.1977. clemisj d, mcgeet: brain stem electric response audiometry in the differential diagnosis of acoustic rumors. laryngoscope, lxxxjx; 31-42,1979. nicolet biomedical a division (4nicolet instrument corporation p.o. box 6 2 7 0 • j o h a n n e s b u r g , south a f r i c a 2 0 0 0 . 6 t h floor, s p e s c o m h o u s e , c o m e r p i o n e e r a n d c e n t r a l r o a d s f o r d s b u r g , j o h a n n e s b u r g 2092. t e l e x : 8 6 2 7 3 . t e l e p h o n e : (011) 834-7551 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28,1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) l a r y n g e a l behaviour and fluency-inducing therapy marlene behrmann ma (speech path.) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg itemed behaviour of an adult stutterer was assessed preand post-therapy intervention. i n t e r v e n t i o n comprised a fluency based ram^directedto the modification ofphonatory behaviour and rate reduction. electrolaryngographic tracings, as well as seventy and t ^ l ratzs vere obtained preail post-intervention. results revealed a marked change in all measurements post-intervention, m^cating the effectiveness of therapy. the 'efficacy of the electrolaryngograph as a measurement tool was illustrated and the results are considered in the light of the current literature on phonatory disturbance in stuttering. teltzfegedrag van >n volwasse hakkelaar is preen post-terapeutiese ingryping ondersoek. ingryping het uit'n vlotheidsgebaseerde ^roe^am ger'g °p d'e ^ s m van fonatoriese gedrag en vermindering van spoed, bestaan. eiektrolaringeografiese op ekeninge asook zoliin o ' die graad en.frelensie van die hakkelgedrag, is voor en na terapeutiese ingryping verkry. die doeltreffendheid van terapfe isbewys deurdat η opvallende verskil t.o.v. alle post-ingrypingsmetings gemerk is. die effektiwiteit van die elektrol^og aaf as meetinstnment is aangetoon en die resultate is teen die agtergrond van die huidige literatuur i. v. m. fonatoriese asking in hakkel bespreek. program is to establish rate control and this dimension together with the emphasis on phonatory adjustments, has currently proved to be the most effective variable handled in treatment (andrews et al. 1982). the stutter free therapy program falls within the 'speak more fluently' school of stuttering therapy, as discussed by gregory (1979). these approaches, also known as the fluency-inducing therapies, advocate the establishment, maintenance and transfer of an entirely new speech pattern. the call for clearer delineation and identification of the factors which contribute to the success or failure of stuttering therapy has been made by boberg, howie and woods (1979) and continued research into the outcome of therapy is necessary both to predict the amount of change and to enhance and modify existing treatment regimes. the concept of an organic etiology of stuttering is not a novel one, stuttering having been ascribed to, among others, neurological causes, auditory perception problems and biochemical factors (van riper 1978). recently, numerous allusions to abnormal laryngeal functioning of stutterers have been made, but whether this abnormal functioning is due to etiological or symptomatological factors is as yet undetermined and may indeed prove unanswerable. several writers have stated that the problem resides in the larynx itself whilst others have suggested that an overriding neural component is responsible for the disturbed phonatory behaviour (conture, mccall and brewer 1977). the stutterer's inability to initiate phonation and to make vocalization adjustments has been examined (adams and reis 1971), as hasjvoice onset time problems (cross and luper 1979) and unusuajl abductory laryngeal behaviour (schwartz 1974). perkins (1981) has concluded that there exists a "mistiming and excessive contraction of laryngeal and supraglottal muscles". one of the difficulties in examining the laryngeal behaviour of stutterers arises from the lack of testing equipment. the development of the electrolaryngograph affords the researcher the opportunity of investigating the phonatory pattern of a speaker in a non-invasive manner (fourcin and abberton 1971). this technique which provides a dynamic, instantaneous and simple method for assessing vocal fold contact and for examining the laryngeal behaviour of stutterers, has not been extensively applied to stuttering nor has it been used to evaluate the laryngeal behaviour preand postintervention. the laryngographic tracings are obtained by placing two electrodes on either side of the thyroid cartilage. the output is derived from the voltage variations which result in the changing conductance of the vocal fold configurations. a potential application of the electrolaryngograph is as a means of examining laryngeal functioning following therapy which has been instituted specifically to modify vocal fold movement. one such therapy program, "stutter free speech" (shames and florance 1980) is designed to modify the phonatory behaviour of the speaker through continuous phonation. a further purpose of this die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 methodology the aim of the current study is to evaluate the change in a stutterer's laryngeal behaviour following a therapy program designed to intervene at the level of phonation. the applicability of the electrolaryngograph as a measurement tool of laryngeal behaviour will also be considered. a single case study design was selected in accordance with perkins (1983) proposal that studies "with an ν of 1" support the tendency away from hypothetical speculation and provide much needed empirical evidence. subject description the subject, s, was a 31 year old male who had stuttered since early childhood. no family history of stuttering had been reported. s had received therapy previously but there had been no observable change in his speech pattern. no previous history of laryngeal pathology had been noted. s presented as aphase lv advanced stutterer, according to luper and m u l d e r s ( 1964) classification system. his speech pattern was charactered by severe blocks and hard contacts, tense repetitions and interjections of phonemic a i d non-phonemic utterances. tension loci were identified as the hps, tongue, laryngeal area and abdomen. secondary behavtours © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 marlene behrmann included eye blinking, head jerks, lip tremor and flaring of the nostrils. respiratory disturbances were evident in frequent pausing, gasping and speaking on residual air. fluency facilitating techniques (utilised prior to the implementation of a therapy program) such as whispering, choral speaking and slowed speech produced a decrease in the frequency and severity of stuttering. s obtained a score of 12.9 on the erikson s24 (1969) scale which suggests that he viewed himself in a positive light prior to intervention. pre-intervention speech assessment the need for comprehensive assessment pre-intervention has been highlighted by andrews, guitar and howie (1981). a preintervention percentage of syllables stuttered (%ss) and a mean syllable per minute (spm) were computed in order to describe the frequency and severity of the stutter pattern. a sample of s's speech was collected including a spontaneous speech sample, a reading sample and a telephone call interchange. these varying conditions were included since bloodstein (1975) contended that the stutter pattern varies under different circumstances. for the purposes of , this study, stuttering was defined as "any sound or syllable repetition, block, prolongation or effortful emission of a word or syllable" (ingham, andrews and winkler 1972). the pre-intervention %ss were 26,5%, 18,2% and31,4% for the three samples outlined above. these figures are suggestive of a severe stutter pattern in comparison with previous studies (goldsmith and anderson 1984) and support the classification of s as a phase iv advanced stutterer. the mean spm counts were 139,5, 132 and 123 for the three samples and are all slow compared with the norm of 196 spm for normal speaking adult males (andrews and ingham 1971), thus confirming the diagnosis of a severe stutter pattern. the electrolaryngographic examination was conducted using a uher 4200 report stereo i.c. tape recorder with a m816 directional microphone to record the subject's spoken output while the mingograf inkjet recorder traced the laryngeal information. the subject was seated in a comfortable position with the microphone at a distance of 30mm from the lips. tracings were made on both a spontaneous speech sample and a reading sample. owing to practical difficulties the tracings could not be carried out during a telephone conversation. the dimensions for analysing the laryngographic tracings were selected to yield a comprehensive picture of laryngeal functioning. examples of the pre-intervention tracings are presented in figures la and lb which reflect the spontaneous speech and reading samples respectively. a. regularity of the wave this dimension refers to the maintenance of a stable vibratory pattern which is achieved through the laryngeal musculature and through air pressure control. much variabil^ ^ ^ ^ — r ^ m i m ^ ^ v w s / w v w · — ivvv-2'!'35 however, the problems involved in accomplishing this goal require no introduction to the speech therapist. the terms generalisation, transfer of training and carry-over have been used interchangeably in the literature to refer to this aspect of therapy. the approachjournal of the south african speech and hearing association. vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mass generalized learning and articulation therapy 31 es that have traditionally been advocated for facilitating this phase of.therapy suggest that most speech therapists have viewed carry-over as the extension,of correct articulation from the clinical setting to other situational settings. inherent in these procedures is the assumption that carry-over is an event,which occurs only in the final stages of therapy. their ineffectiveness is reflected in a statement made by mowrer.21 'carry-over remains one of the most perplexing problems speech clinicians have to face in attempting to modify articulation.' since the problems of carry-over continue to plague speech therapists it is fitting that research be directed towards scrutinising the efficacy of traditional therapy procedures and to designing alternative procedures with the sole purpose of eliminating carry-over difficulties.4 it is to this aim that the present study was directed. recently morehead and johnson20 made observations that have shed a new light on the carry-over controversy. they attribute the unstable changes that ; occur in therapy to the learning theory framework in which most speech therapy programmes take place. raymore25 notes that most speech therapists use the basic "stimulus method" of therapy t a teach correct production. this technique has its origin in the early work of travis (1931) but the procedure formulated by van riper in 1939 is frequently referred to as the "traditional approach" to articulation therapy. in most cases the therapist presents a model of the correct sound and the child is asked to repeat it. reinforcement is provided for each successful attempt on approximation to success. morehead and johnson20 note that this pairing of a stimulus and response constitutes a specific training experience, the consequences of which are unstable changes in the newly learned behaviour. . in contrast piagetian-based programmes.'.. assume that mass generalised experience is crucial to learning and these should provide more stable changes in behaviour. many enthusiasts in different fields have adapted piaget's viewpoint to suit their specific needs. since no further references to this method of teaching could be found which is specifically applied to speech therapy, the writer interpreted this principle in a way that was thought best suited to articulation learning. impetus for change in the nature of articulation therapy has also resulted from the application of linguistic theory to normal and defective articulation. recent research has resulted in the proposal that articulation problems be considered in the framework of the phonological s y s t e m . 3 ' 4 ' 1 4 ' 1 9 ' 2 2 ' 2 3 ' 3 1 as a number of phonemes share features in common it has been thought that it is the omission or misuse of a feature that accounts for the misarticulation errors of the child. the implication is that correction of articulation must be directed at locating and correcting the feature which is common to a number of misarticulations.18'19'23 thus traditional therapy procedures of teaching sounds in isolation are being substituted for by procedures in which an entire category of sounds is worked o n . 1 3 ' 3 1 it is proposed that these recent developments in the remediation of multiple articulation defects can be utilised to translate the principle of mass generalised tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir sprak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 avril klaff learning experience into a practical therapy procedure for dealing with a group of children having difficulty with only one phoneme. it was hypothesized that emphasis on the entire category of sounds of which the target phoneme is a member will provide the child with more generalised learning experience than the teaching of a single, isolated sound. this in turn will result in more stable changes in articulatory ability, hence eliminating carry-over difficulties. method aim it was the aim of the present study to employ the principle of mass generalised learning experience in articulation therapy and to observe its effects on the entire therapeutic process. subjects: four subjects (ss) were divided into two groups of two ss each. the experimental ss were trained according to the principle of mass generalised learning. the control ss received traditional articulation therapy. all ss were selected according to the following criteria: 1. chronological age of 8 years or more in order to eliminate developmental articulation errors. 2. presence of an interdental lisp. 3. hearing within normal limits. since great difficulty was experienced in obtaining ss who conformed to these criteria, those available had to be matched as closely as possible according to the following variables: subject c.a. sex articulation error etiological factors previous therapy el 7,6 yrs. f θ is biz l/r none observable none ci ' 9,6 yrs. μ 0/s b/z m φ none observable none e2 8,0 yrs. μ 0/s &/z tongue thrust emotional problems. learning problems. none / c2 9,6 yrs. f 0/s &/z tongue thrust emotional problems. learning problems. a few sessions at 4 yrs. table 1. criteria according to which subjects were matched. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23. december 976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mass generalized learning and articulation therapy 33 age since the ages of ss ranged from 7,6 years to 9,6 years, the experimenter (e) felt that group therapy would be enhanced by placing children of the same age in the same group. therefore, experimental and control ss were not matched according to this variable. sex one male and one female was placed in each group although this variable is not considered crucial.34 articulation error el (experimental s 1) and ci (control s 1) had additional errors. (see table 1) etiological factors e2 (experimental s 2) and c2 (control s2) were matched for the presence of a tongue thrust swallow pattern and suspected emotional difficulties both of which may be related to articulation disorders.16'34 intelligence ss were not matched according to l.q. since this is not felt to be causally related to articulation disorders.34 however ss were matched according to an informal assessment of learning ability. procedure tests for the effects of therapy: , figure 1 shows the sequence of therapy with the relative position of the testing sequence indicated. the tests were administrated on a before and after therapy basis as well as at specific intervals during therapy. pre-therapy test: > a. goldman ftistoe b. competence tests c. baseline probes * intra-therapy probes final position training post-therapy test: goldman fristoe b. spectrographic analysis c. 24 hi. retention probe d. 3 week retention probe figure i. the sequence of therapy with the relative position of the testing sequence indicated. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 avril klaff pre-therapy tests these were administrated to determine the level of performance for each s prior to the commencement of therapy. these included the goldman fristoe test of articulation8 arid two competence tests devised by the ε to determine if ss were able to make the phonological and phonetic distinctions between the target and error sound. thus ss were required to discriminate between minimal pairs containing [ + strident] sounds and [ strident] sounds as well as minimal pairs containing slit and grooved phonemes. in addition the acoustic spectrograph was used to analyse the ss renditions of the /s/ phoneme in an attempt to characterise more accurately the acoustic properties of the pretherapy and post-therapy articulations. intra-therapy probe tests l the probe tests used in the present study were based on those used by elbert, she it on and arndt,5 shelton, elbert and arndt2 7 and wright, shelton and arndt.35 the tests can be divided into two categories. a. sound production tasks (spts) a 40 item spt was divided into 20 items for the /s/ phoneme and 20 items for the /z/ phoneme. both phonemes were sampled in initial, medial final positions and in clusters. b. carry-over tasks generalisation of correct production to spontaneous speech was measured in four ways: 1. subjects were asked to read aloud until 20 instances of the /s/ and /z/ phonemes had been sampled. 2. spontaneous speech was tape recorded in the therapy setting. 3. spontaneous speech was recorded in an extra-therapy setting, namely the grounds of the university. 4. parents were given some understanding of how to observe the child's speech in different situations and were asked to report carry-over progress to the e. all the intra-therapy probes were administered prior to therapy in order to establish baseline performance levels for each s. the sequence of administration of these probes during therapy was not uniform for all ss since they all progressed at difference rates. probe tests were administered when a s obtained a score of 18/20 at the end of each phase of therapy.,if scores indicated that generalisation had taken place to the next level of production then that phase of therapy was omitted. post-therapy tests 1. the goldman-fristoe test of articulation8 was re-administered 24 hours after the termination of therapy. 2. both spts and carry-over tasks were re-administered 24 hours and again 3 weeks following the termination of therapy. 3. spectrographic analyses were made of each ss pre-therapy and posttherapy renditions of 7 /s/ and /z/ words from the goldman-fristoe test of articulation.8 journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23 december 976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mass generalized learning and articulation therapy 35 reliability random samples of recordings of probe tests were played to two experienced speech therapists. the ε rated the probes at the time of recording and on playback. therapy procedures each group was seen for approximately 30 minutes, five days a week. therapy was terminated when complete carry-over had been achieved or when the maximum number of 20 sessions had been reached. discrimination training the initial stages of the experimental therapy procedure were aimed at establishing the distinction between the two groups of contrasting sounds. thus all the [ + strident] or grooved sounds (/s/, /z/, /s/, /z/, /ts / and /dz/ according to halle10 were said to belong to "the man with the tall hat". the [ strident] or slit sounds (/f/, /v/, /0/ and (/£/) according to halle10 were identified with the "man with the short hat". discrimination exercises were carried out in which the children were required to associate minimal pair words with the relevant "men". in contrast, control therapy was concerned with identifying the error and the target sound (/0/"and /s/) and training the children to discriminate between these two sounds.30 the acquisition phase the experimental ss were taught production of all the [ + strident] sounds. production of /s/ and /z/ was taught by emphasizing the auditory and visual similarities between these sounds and other f + strident] sounds. the ss were asked to consciously contrast [ + strident] and [ strident] sounds in their own speech so that the differences could be emphasized. the correct production of all [ + strident] sounds was stressed at all levels of the acquisition phase. this differs markedly from the traditional procedures in which the child's attention is focused solely on correct production of the phoneme /s/. the carry-over phase this phase was provided for ss who failed to demonstrate carry-over following the acquisition phase of therapy. figure 1 indicates that this phase was divided into two stages. experimental and control procedures differed in that experimental ss were encouraged to use all [ + strident] sounds while the attention of the control ss was focused only on the consistent use of /s/. results and discussion the small number of ss used in the study did not justify a statistical analysis of the results and data are therefore presented in terms of individual performances. comparison of pre-therapy and post-therapy test results a. competence tests results indicated that all ss were able to discriminate on both a phonetic and phonological level, therefore all had competence for correct production. b. the goldman fristoe test of articulation — all ss were found to consistently misarticulate the /s/ and /z/ phonemes prior to therapy. all were consistent in correct production of these two phonemes on the tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23, desember 1976. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 avril klaff post-therapy test seemingly indicative of the fact that all ss had achieved the same level of performance. however, results of the carry-over tasks administered at the conclusion of therapy showed that only one s (el) had attained consistent use of /s/ and /z/ in all spontaneous speech situations while the remaining three ss demonstrated inconsistent carry-over. c. spectrograph^ analysis the spectrograms were analysed with the aid of a phonetician. a systematic variation between pre-therapy and posttherapy renditions was observed. 1. the lowest frequency at which peaks of energy occur shifted from 2000hz to 3500hz. 2. weighting of energy shifted from the mid-frequencies to the higher frequencies. 3. peaks of energy extended to 16 000hz. the accoustic patterns of the pre-therapy articulations do not conform to those reported to be characteristic of any other english fricatives. thus the misarticulation does not appear to be one of substitution of/#/ for /s/ but could be more adequately described as an "articulatory displacement".9 however, the post-therapy patterns correspond closely to the patterns apparent in the e's spectrogram confirming that the ss had achieved acceptable production as a result of therapy. intra-therapy probe test r e s u l t s the results presented in this section document the changes that occurred in articulation generalisation as each s progressed through the programme. a. sound production tasks table ii shows the total number of correct responses on the word and sentence task for each s. the following trends can be extracted from table ii. 1. discrimination training minimally improved production. a number of researchers have questioned the excessive reliance of therapy programmes on discrimination training.6'1 5;1 7' ' ' 2. the scores on the /s/spt exceeded scores obtained on the /z/spt for all ss during word training. thus the benefits of training the experimental ss in correct production of /s/ and /z/ simultaneously was not evident. however, while all four ss obtained maximum scores on the /s/spt, only the experimental ss scored maximally on the /z/spt. (see table ii). the ε postulates that the effects of mass generalised learning experience may have begun to manifest itself in the ease with which the experimental ss generalised correct production from words to sentences. it was observed that the control ss produced the stimulus word correctly but had difficulty generalising correct production to other target phonemes in the sentence. the control ss were oriented towards a one-to-one relationship between response and reinforcement whereas the experimental ss were rewarded for a diversity of responses. the latter journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23 december 976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mass generalized learning and articulation therapy 37 appears to reduce the child's focus on specific stimuli. rather, the child is made aware of the generality of correct production. subject probe phase of therapy baseline discrim. i f μ sent. sp.sp. pi p2 ei is/ spt 0 1 17 18 / 20 / 20 20 ei /z/spt 0 0 11 14 / 20 / 20 20 ci isi spt 0 0 18 / 20 20 20 ci /z/spt 0 0 19 / 15 20 20' e2 is/ spt 0 0 6 16 / 20 20 19 e2 /z/ spt 0 0 0 13 / 20 20 20 c2 isl spt 0 3 18 / 20 20 20 c2 /z/spt 0 0 10 / 16 20 18 discrim. = discrimination training 1 / 1 = no therapy indicated sp.sp. = spontaneous speech — = probe not administered pi = 24 hour retention test | = termination of therapy p2 = 3 week retention test table ii. total number of correct articulations for all subjects on sound production tasks. b. carry-over tasks in order to afford a comparative analysis the graphs of matched experimental and control ss will be compared. comparison between el and ci (see figure 2) el and ci obtained similar results on the reading tasks and intra-therapy talking task. however, scores on the extra-therapy talking tasks differed for these two ss. el showed spontaneous generalisation to extra-therapy situations following the sentence phase of therapy. ci failed to demonstrate generalisation to these contexts until correct production had been reinforced in extra-therapy settings. parental reports confirmed that el had attained complete carry-over to all speaking situations and consequently she was dismissed at the end of the sentence phase of therapy. ci retained 100% scores on the retention tests. after having been subjected to carry-over therapy ci remained inconsistent jn his generalisation of correct production to the extratherapy talking task. further, the results of the three week retention test suggested that ci had regressed rather than improved in his carry-over ability. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 avril klaff 18' 17in « uj 0) 15-ζ ο μtη subject e1 / s / r t r t -itt -ett = baseline = discrimination = i n i t i a l position = f i n a l position = m e d i a l position = s e n t e n c e s = c a r r y o v e r in t h e clinic coc = c a r r y over o u t s i d e t h e clinic pi = 24 hour r e t e n t i o n t e s t p2 = 3 week r e t e n t i o n t e s t r t = r e a d i n g t a s k itt = i n t r a t h e r a p y t a l k i n g task ett = e x t r a t h e r a p y t a l k i n g task 10 11 ,12 13, 14 15 16 17 1θ 19 20 ~~' s cic p1 i f μ therapy phase f & m s cic t h e r a p y phase figure 2: performance of el and ci on reading tasks and talking tasks at each phase of therapy. comparison between e2 and c2 (see figure 3) e2 did not attain the 50% criteria at the end of the initial position phase of therapy for the administration of carry-over tasks. c2 attained this criteria but failed to demonstrate any carry-over at all. e2's carry-over to spontaneous speech was investigated at the end of initial position training and the data indicates a greater degree of generalisation than c2 had shown to the initial carry-over tasks. journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23 december 976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mass generalized learning and articulation therapy 39 e2's generalisation to the reading tasks and the intra-therapy talking task was similar to that of c2. however, e2 showed some spontaneous generalisation to the extra-therapy talking task at the end of the sentence phase of therapy while c2, like ci, failed to show generalisation to these contexts until correct production had been reinforced in spontaneous speech. e2's scores on the extra-therapy talking task exceeded those of c2 at all levels of therapy. neither of these ss attained complete carry-over before termination and both showed regression following the three week break. f μ s therapy phase coc pi p2 = baseline = discrimination = i n i t i a l position = f i n a l position = m e d i a l position = s e n t e n c e s = c a r r y o v e r in t h e clinic coc = c a r r y o v e r outside the clinic = 24 hour r e t e n t i o n test = 3 week r e t e n t i o n test = r e a d i n g task = i n t r a t h e r a p y talking task e t t = e x t r a t h e r a p y talking task i t 2 3 4. .5 6 7. θ .9 10 ii 12 .13 14 15. .16 17 18 19. 20 β d i fsm s cic coc p1 p2 therapy phase figure 3: performance ofe2 and c2 on reading tasks and talking tasks at each phase of therapy. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 avril klaff a general trend emerges from the close comparison of the intra-therapy probe test results of matched experimental and control ss. in the initial acquisition stages of therapy the scores of the control ss exceeded that of their matched experimental counterparts. however, this situation reversed itself starting at the sentence phase of therapy. of significance are the greater scores obtained by the experimental ss on the extra-therapy talking task. although a limitation of the present study is a lack of accurate assessment of carry-over to nonclinic environments, these scores appeared to demonstrate that both experimental ss were readily able to generalise correct production to situations unrelated to speech therapy. reliability of data (see table iii) phase of rating r1 and r2 rl, r2 and e2 el and e2 discrimination 59,4% 46,9% 91,2% initial pos. 90,6% 69,4% 80,0% 24 hr. retention 90,0% 87,8% 96,0% r1 and r2 rating between 2 independent judges rl, r2 and e2 agreement between raters and the experimenter el and e2 — agreement between 2 sets of measurements made by the experimenter table iii. mean percentage of inter-judge and intra-judge reliability. the poor reliability scores found in this study seem to-indicate that listener judgement alone is inadequate due to the acoustic similarity between /0/ and /s/. the use of visual cues as in videotaping and a more objective measure such as spectrographic analysis could eradicate some of the uncertainty. general discussion a number of factors have been said to be important in determining "an individual's ability to modify his behaviour.29 the unique characteristics of each ss progress suggests that these factors must be taken into account when examining the efficacy of a therapeutic technique. a limitation of this study was the number of inter-subject variables that had to be controlled for. the results of this study suggest that the presence of a tongue thrust did not impede e2 and c2'slability to acquire correct production these results support mason and proffit's16 contention that the speech defect can be corrected without prior correction of the tongue thrust. tongue thrust does not appear to be related to carry-over problems since.ci experienced the journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23. december 976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mass generalized learning and articulation therapy 41 same difficulty. however, the influence of motivational factors, differences in chronological age and variations in learning ability may have contributed to the differing rates of progress observed. the effect of certain therapeutic variables was controlled for since all ss were seen for intensive group therapy. since the ε was the therapist for both groups, the role of therapist variables and experimenter bias cannot be excluded. despite these confounding variables, it is important to ask what factors inherent in the principle of mass generalised learning may have accounted for the finding that el showed spontaneous carry-over of correct production. the following tentative explanation is offered on the basis of certain concepts taken from piagetian and phonological literature. the writer postulates that 'grouping' based on distinctive feature similarity may serve to provide the child with a permanent system of articulatory monitoring. the grouping together of sounds closely related to one another provides the child with the opportunity of discerning the relationship between sounds already in his repertoire namely /s/, /z/, /ts/ and /dz/ and the target phonemes is/ and /z/. studies have shown that the child improves his articulation of phonemes that share distinctive features with the phoneme being taught. 1 8 ' 3 3 similarly, it is possible that the child will generalise the properties of the sounds he is already able to produce to those sounds that are in error. distinctive feature classes are reported to have corresponding clusters of sensory and motor feedback networks.28 thus, when the child attempts production of /s/ and /z/ correspondence to the feedback patterns associated with /s/, /z/, /ts/ and /dz/ would be a signal to the child that his attempts were successful. it is hypothesized that if this internal referent system is established for the child in the early stages of therapy, carry-over of correct production to speech contexts unrelated to the speech therapy situation will be facilitated. the efficiency of this procedure may reside in its ability to provide the child with an independent system of articulatory monitoring. herein lies the major downfall of traditional procedures. the result of their focus on the stimulusresponse reinforcement paradigm is that the child is dependent on the therapist to monitor and reinforce his attempts at correct production. mass generalised learning experience, however, seems to provide the child with a strategy for coping with correct production regardless of the presence or absence of stimuli related to correct production in the clinic. conclusion despite the limitations and difficulties inherent in this type of study, the results do provide some support for morehead and johnson's20 hypothesis and also seem to suggest that the application of this principle in the field of articulation therapy may offer the speech therapist the advantage of naturally and effectively attaining successful carry-over. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23 desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 avril klaff references 1. backus, 0 . and dunn, h.m. (1947): intensive group therapy in speech rehabilitation./. speech dis., 12, 2, 135-142. 2. carrell, j.a. (1968): disorders of articulation. prentice-hall inc., englewood cliffs, n.j. 3. compton, a.j. (1970): generative studies of children's phonological disorders. 7. speech hear. dis., 35, 4, 315-399. 4. crocker, j.r. (1969): a phonological model of children's articulation competence. / speech hear. dis., 34, 3, 203-213. 5., elbert, m. shelton r.l. and arndt, w.b. (1967): a task for the evaluation of articulation change: i. the development of methodology./. speech hear. res. 10,2,281-288. 6. ' fry, d.b. (1968): the phonemic system of children's speech. british j. disorders communication. 3, 1, 13-19. 7. goda, s. (1970): articulation therapy and consonant drill book: grune and stratton, n.y. 8. goldman, r. and fristoe, m. (1969): goldman-fristoe test of ariculation. american guidance service inc., minnesota. 9. guile, t.c. (1975): personal communication.oept. of phonetics and linguistics, univ. of witwatersrand. 10. halle, m. (1964): on the bases of phonology. in fodor, j.a. and katz, j.j. (eds.) the structure of language: readings in the philosophy of language. prentice-hall inc., englewood cliffs, n.y. 11. hall-powers, m. (1971): functional disorders of articulation: symptomatology and etiology. chapter 33 in handbook of speech pathology and audiology. travis, l.e. (ed.) appleton-century crofts, n.y. 12. hall-powers, m. (1971): clinical and educational procedures in functional disorders of articulation. chapter 34 in handbook of speech pathology and audiology. travis, l.e. (ed.) appleton-century crofts, n.y. 13. hannah, e.p. standel, j. and gardner, j. (1974): distinctive feature analysis. chapter 7 in applied linguistic analysis, hannah, e.p. (ed.) joyce publications, california. 14. leonard, l.b. (1973): the nature of deviant articulation./. speech hear. dis., 38,2, 156-161. 15. lewis, f.c. (1974): distinctive feature confusions in production and discrimination of selected consonants. language and speech, 17,1, 60-67. 16. mason, r.n. and proffit, w.r.: the tongue thrust controversy: background and recommendations. /. speech hear. dis., 39, 2,' 115-132. 17. mcclean, j.e. (1970): extending stimulus control of phoneme articulation by operant techniques. a.s.h.a. monographs, 14, 4, 2447. 18. mcreynolds, l.v. and bennet, s. (1972): distinctive feature generalisation and articulation training. /. speech hear. dis., 37, 4, 462470. 19. mcreynolds, l.v.' and huston, k. (1971): a distinctive feature analysis of children's misarticulations./. speech hear. dis., 36, 155-166. 20. morehead, d.m. and johnson, m. (1972): piaget's theory of intelligence applied to the assessment and treatment of linguistically deviant journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23. december / 976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mass generalized learning and articulation therapy 43 children. papers and reports on child language development, 4, 143-161. 21. mowrer, d.e. (1971): the transfer of training in articulation therapy. /. speech hear. dis., 36, 4, 427446. 22. oiler, d.k. (1970): regularities in abnormal child phonology. j. speech hear. dis., 38, 3647. 23. pollack, e. and rees, n.s. (1972): disorders of articulation: some clinical application of a distinctive feature approach./. speech hear. dis., 37,4,451461. 24. prins, d. (1963): relations among specific articulatory deviations and responses to a clinical measure of sound discrimination./. speech hear. dis., 28, 4, 382-388. 25. raymore, s. (1970): the effects of systematic training programs on the generalisation of new phoneme responses across different positions in words. unpublished master's thesis, kansas state university. 26. shafer, g. (1974): a spectrographs investigation of acoustic cues in reduced consonant clusters in normal and abnormal child speech. unpublished research report, dept. of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand. 27. shelton, r.l. elbert, m. and arndt, w.b. (1967): a task for the evaluation of articulation change. ii. comparison of task scores during baseline and lesson series testing. /. speech hear. res., 10, 3, 578-585. 28. sommers, r.k. and kane, a.r. (1974): nature and remediation of functional articulation disorders. chapter 3 in communication disorders: remedial principles and practices. dickson, s.e. (ed.) scott, foresman and co., illinois. 29. van demark, d.r. (1971): articulatory changes in the therapeutic process. cleft pal. j., 8, 159-165. 30. van riper, c. (1972): speech correction: principles and methods. (5th edition). prentice-hall, inc., englewood cliffs, ν j : 31. weber, j.l. (1970): patterning of deviant articulation behaviour. /. speech hear. dis., 35,2, 135-141. 32. weiner, p.s. (1967): auditory discrimination and articulation. /. speech hear. dis., 32, 1, 19-28. 33. winitz, h. and priesler, l. (1967): the effect of distinctive feature pretraining in phoneme discrimination learning./. speech hear. res., 10,3,515-530. 34. winitz, h. (1969): articulatory acquisition and behaviour. appletoncentury crofts, n.y. 35. wright, v. shelton, r.l. and arndt, w.b. (1969): a task for the evaluation of articulation change: iii, imitative task scores compared with scores for more spontaneous tasks. /. speech hear. res., 12, 4, 875-884. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acoustics and signal analysis noise problems? general radio has the solution: a complete family of noise measuring instruments for community and industrial noise: n gr1933 — 9 precision sound-level meter gr1944 noise-exposure indicator and monitor gr1565-b for more information please contact: associated electronics (pty.) ltd. p.o. box 31094, braamfontein, phone 839-1824 150 caroline street, b r i x t o n , johannesburg. journal of the south african speech and hearing association. vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 73 the teaching of blissymbols as a bridge into literacy for children with cognitive impairments: a comparison of two training approaches enid moolman and erna alant centre for augmentative and alternative communication, department of communication pathology, university of pretoria summary this study compares the learning of blissymbols by six mildly cognitively impaired children by means of a global and an analytic approach. training consisted of two stages. the first was the training of eight compound symbols and the second the training of seven single configuration symbols. the study concludes that blissymbols as an entrance into literacy can be taught successfully to cognitively impaired individuals by means of either an analytic or a global approach. the analytic approach seems to have greater long-term benefits, as the subjects instructed by the analytic approach consistently performed better in the generalization and re-evaluation procedures. the analytic approach, however, was much more time consuming than the global approach in terms of the length of training required. the implications for literacy development and augmentative and alternative communication systems are discussed. opsomming hierdie studie vergelyk, deur middel van 'n globale en 'n analitiese benadering, die aanleer van blissimbole deur ses kinders elk met geringe kognitiewe gestremdheid. opleiding het uit twee fases bestaan, waarvan die eerste die opleiding van agt saamgestelde simbole behels het en die tweede die opleiding van sewe enkel-element simbole. die bevindinge dui daarop dat bliss-simbole suksesvol deur individue met geringe kognitiewe gestremdheid aangeleer kan word deur middel van of 'n globale of'n analitiese benadering, alhoewel die verskille tussen resultate nie statistics betekenisvol is nie. dit het egter geblyk dat die analitiese opleidingsmetode sekere langtermynvoordele ingehou het, aangesien daar 'n konstante tendens vir die analitiese groep was om beter te vaar met die veralgemeningsen die herevaluasie-prosedures. wat betref die lengte van opleiding was die analitiese opleidingsmetode egter baie meer tydrowend as die globale metode. die implikasies vir die ontwikkeling van geletterdheid en aanvullende en alternatiewe kommunikasie is bespreek. key words: blissymbols, literacy, global approach, analytic approach j introduction j i for many years it was believed that cognitively impaired individuals cannot acquire any literacy skills as they seem to be unable to learn to read and write. studies by researchers such as sterick (1979), folk and campbell (1978), and raver and dwyer (1986), however, demonstrate that the cognitively impaired child can, in fact, learn to read and write. nevertheless, it is worth noting that cognitively impaired individuals tend to achieve reading levels that are below their cognitive levels (buttery & creekmore, 1985). the latter fact has led to a re-evaluation of instructional approaches for teaching literacy skills to the cognitively impaired population as well as our definition of literacy. as a result, the use of augmentative and alternative communication strategies as a bridge to literacy has come under scrutiny. many view literacy in a restricted sense, namely as the ability to read and write. in contrast with this limited view, miller (1990:2) states that "... becoming literate involves learning how to manipulate various bodies of knowledge ... and because being literate involves a way of thinking, literacy becomes entwined with how and what people know. one who is literate is conversant with what is going on in the world." two models of teaching literacy skills can therefore be distinguished. we can either teach the child the technical skill of reading and writing (model 1), or social skills, in other words, equip the child to become a literate participant in society (model 2). the problem with teaching the child only the technical skills of reading/writing is that this is neither meaningful nor functional and the child is unable to identify with it. literacy can be viewed as a systematic progression from recognizing pictures to the use of an abstract symbol system such as normal orthography. to become literate, an individual must be visually directed, able to extract meaning, and able to interpret symbols on different levels of abstractness. initially, blissymbols were used primarily to augment speech in individuals with severe speech and communication problems. it was soon realized, however, that blissymbols could play an important role in bridging the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 enid moolman & erna alant gap between pictures and print, as blissymbols are concept-based and thus easier to learn than normal orthography (luftig & bersani, 1985). in addition, the child can be prepared for the processing of print through the use of blissymbols, as their use includes many of the skills needed for normal orthography. the ability to synthesize elements to make a word, and the ability to integrate and process a number of symbols that communicate an idea are important elements in preparing a child for reading (archer, 1977). these skills can be facilitated by the use of blissymbols, depending on the nature of the instructional approach. as in the teaching of reading, two main approaches for the teaching of blissymbols can be identified, namely a global and an analytic approach. a global approach entails training of the entire symbol without any reference to the symbol elements. an analytic approach, on the other hand, requires an explanation of the meaning of each of the elements within a compound symbol. if a child is therefore taught blissymbols by means of an analytic approach, he/she might be able to transfer this analytic skill to normal orthography (alant, 1994; mcnaughton, 1993). shepherd and haaf(1992) compared the global and analytic training approaches by training individuals who were not disabled, and they found that an analytic training approach could have several advantages over a global approach. results obtained on cognitively normal individuals, however, cannot simply be transferred to the cognitively impaired population. the research discussed in this article therefore serves as a preliminary study to investigate the effects of the global and analytic approaches to blissymbol training for cognitively impaired individuals. it is important for us to determine the effect of these two training approaches as it might be significant in our clinical application. method aims the primary aim of this study was to compare two training approaches in the teaching of blissymbols to cognitively impaired children. the objectives of this study were: to teach specific symbols to group 1 by means of a global approach to teach the same symbols to group 2 by means of an analytic approach to compare the results obtained by these two groups with reference to: * the number of presentations required to reach the criterion * the number of minutes required to reach the criterion * the % of symbols correctly analysed and synthesized during a generalization procedure * the % of symbols correctly identified after a set period without any exposure (re-evaluation procedure) to do a qualitative analysis of individual subject's performances in symbol acquisition with regard to: * ease of acquisition * nature and complexity of symbols subjects pupils enrolled at a school for cognitively impaired children were selected according to specified selection criteria. a total of eight subjects was initially selected, but due to numerous absences and poor co-operation (which, it was felt, could contribute to invalid test results), subjects 1 and 8 were dropped from the study. six subjects (4 male, 2 female) were thus used in this study. all subjects presented with mild cognitive impairment, with an iq score of between 50 and 65, and their chronological ages ranged from 7 to 10 years. to check that, subjects were unable to read the blissymbols, subjects who had a functional reading vocabulary of less than 40 words were selected and subjects had to have had no prior familiarity with blissymbols. all subjects' speech was intelligible most of the time in a context unknown to the listener. none of the subjects suffered from visual, speech, hearing or obvious emotional problems that would have interfered with their performance in the tasks required in this study. all subjects were native afrikaans speakers and came from families who enjoyed middle to high socio-economic status (moolman, 1994). subject characteristics are outlined in table 1. subject selection procedures all subjects had to comply with the criteria previously discussed. for the initial selection of the eight subjects, a pre-evaluation of certain skills was administered (see table 2 and appendix d for a detailed outline). subjects were then paired with one another according to the results obtained during the pre-evaluation. general level of functioning (which included iq score) as judged by the teacher and the local occupational therapist, was considered to be an important aspect for the pairing process. subjects were further paired according to their receptive language, visual perception and reading skills. subject 4 was paired with subject 2, subject 3 with 7 and subject 5 with 6. after subjects had been paired, they were randomly divided into two groups, with each group having the same number of subjects, with more or less the same level of skills. subjects in group 1 (subjects 3, 4, 5) were trained by means of a global training approach and subjects from group 2 (subjects 7, 6, 2) by means of an analytic training approach. table 2 provides a description of the paired subjects' performances on the different aspects used for pairing. research design . a quasi-experimental design was selected for this study as a tightly controlled experimental design in which two groups would be totally comparable would be impossible due to the diversity of children with cognitive impairments. for the purpose of this study six children were identified (three in a group). each child in group 1 was paired with another in group 2. although pairing was a difficult process within this context, the teachers and therapists at the school agreed that on a functional level these two groups were comparable. material and apparatus used during the pre-eval uation phase the tests and material used to assess the subjects' reading, visual perception, level of functioning and receptive language during the pre-evaluation, are outlined in table 3. all the tests and material used during the pre-evaluation are, however, not discussed here due to restricted the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the teaching of blissymbols as a bridge into literacy for children with cognitive impairments 75 t a b l e 1. subject characteristics characteristics subject 2 (group 2) subject 3 (group 1) subject 4 (group 1) subject 5 (group 1) subject 6 (group 2) subject 7 (group 2) chronological age (years and months) 8,5 8,6 9,3 7,3 7,6 8,11 socio-economic status middle-high middle-high high middle high middle hearing normal normal normal normal normal normal vision: general description adequate glasses, adequate adequate short-sighted, adequate adequate glasses, adequate gender male female male female male male home language afrikaans afrikaans afrikaans afrikaans afrikaans afrikaans intelligence quotient* 59 (1991) 50 (1991) 64 (1990) 63 (1992) 64 (1992) 55 (1991) diagnosis cognitive impairment down syndrome microcephaly diffuse brain damage cognitive impairment cognitive impairment medical problems no epilepsy no epilepsy no epilepsy no epilepsy no epilepsy no epilepsy prior exposure to bliss none none none none none none emotional state no obvious problems no obvious problems no obvious problems no obvious problems no obvious problems minimal autistic key to table 1: group 1: subjects taught by global training approach group 2: subjects taught by analytic training approach chronological age: the age recorded in years and months at the commencement of the pre-evaluation phase * intelligence quotient: the year in which the iq test was administered is included in brackets. table 2. criteria used for pairing subjects criteria used for pairing pair 1 1 pair 2 pair 3 subjects s2 (group 2) ί s4 (group 1) s7 (group 2) s3 (group 1) s6 (group 2) s5 (group 1) visual perception good averagegood good good average good average reading (average %) 86 75 76 78 18 17 level of functioning mild cognitive impairment mild cognitive impairment mild cognitive impairment mild cognitive impairment mild cognitive impairment mild cognitive impairment level of functioning: iq score 59 64 50 52 64 63 receptive language (months) 54 1 52 50 60 61 48 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 enid moolman & erna alant space. results obtained during thhe pre-evaluation are provided in appendix d. training material and procedure blissymbols were selected from hehner's (1980) "blissymbols for use" as well as from mcdonald's (1989) "teaching and using blissymbolics". a total of twenty eight symbols that represent everyday concepts were selected. eight compound symbols were selected for training during stage 1 and seven single configuration symbols for training during stage 2. a total of fifteen symbols plus three practise items (ball, flower and chair) were thus included for training. symbols for the training phase were selected to include elements of the compound symbols included in the generalization procedure. ten symbols were carefully selected for the generalization procedure (refer to appendix c). the aim of this procedure was to determine whether subjects were able to analyze, synthesize and interpret new symbols based on knowledge gained during training. the symbol elements of the stimuli used in the generalization procedure were all introduced during training, but in combination with different elements: single configuration as well as compound symbols were selected from different word categories (nouns, verbs, adjectives). single configuration symbols can be defined as symbols that contain only one semantic unit or element, e.g., "house", "wheel" and "cloth". compound symbols refer to combinations of more than one semantic element to form new meaning. the symbols that were used during training as well as for the generalization procedure are listed in the appendix. pictures and/or objects representing the eighteen symbols (fifteen training plus three practise items) selected for training were used to explain the concepts and to facilitate training. games and apparatus which included a bliss-man, bliss-electro, a contextual storyboard, matching symbol-to-picture worksheets, picture-your-bliss table 3. tests and material used during the pre-evaluation phase areas tested person responsible for administering the tests tests/material used processing of data reading researcher in co-operation with classroom teacher flashcards containing words which had been taught during the year, were used. areas of evaluation: class mates' names (recognition and labelling) functional reading (recognition and labelling) sight words (recognition and labelling) an average score was determined which was derived from the results obtained for recognition as well as labelling. the percentage referred to in table 2 is thus the mean percentage recognized and labelled correctly in the three areas of evaluation visual local developmental test of visual perception (frostig, 1963) draw-a-man test (goodenough, 1926) developmental test for visualmotor integration (beery, 1967) subjective evaluation: * visual memory * sequencing skills an average score which included all of the subtests, was obtained. visual perceptual skills were categorized as good or average-good i level of functioning iq score school psychologist of that particular district unknown most of the children were tested in the year that they turned six. the year in which the iq test was administered is provided in table 1. iq scores were felt to be current, as the experiment was conducted in 1993 and most of the iq tests performed in 1991 and 1992 / receptive language researcher afrikaans translation of tacl-r (kritzinger, 1985) subtests: word classes grammatical morphemes elaborated sentences a mean score was derived from the results obtained on the three subtests the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the teaching of blissymbols as a bridge into literacy for children with cognitive impairments 77 (blissymbolics communication institute, 1984), dice, puzzles and snap cards were used during the training stage. colour-coded flash cards of all the symbols were made according to the guidelines provided by mcdonald (1989). a weekly outline of the data collection procedures is provided in table 4. prior to training, a pre-evaluation of various skills (language, speech, visual perception, vision, hearing and reading) was performed (see appendix d for results). a pilot study was performed prior to the main study in order to test the effectiveness of the procedure and materials. recommendations were made on the basis of the results of the pilot study. please refer to moolman (1994) for a detailed discussion of the results. the concepts were evaluated on the day before training started. concepts that were unknown were explained, until the subject(s) reached 100%. a baseline evaluation was also performed on this day in order to determine the subjects' ability to guess the meaning of the symbols. training consisted of two stages. stage 1 concentrated on the training of compound symbols and stage 2 on the training of single configuration symbols. compound symbols were trained first, as the researcher wanted to start with two distinct approaches. if single configuration symbols were trained first, the subjects of group 2's actual orientation towards the compound symbols might have been influenced negatively. the presentation of the seven single configuration symbols was the same for both the analytic and global groups. presentation of the compound symbols, however, differed. training was performed by the researcher on a daily basis for approximately 30 minutes per group. this procedure was continued until the 100% criterion for each stage was met by all the subjects. subjects who met this criterion were still included in the training, in order to keep the time without exposure to symbols the same for all subjects. this was an important factor in the re-evaluation procedure. games and activities were used during each training session. picturejyour-bliss was used on days 1, 2 and 3 of stage 1, and only on day 1 of stage 2 of the training. j experimental group 1 (subjects 3,4,5) was trained by means of a global approach. the global training approach implied visual recognition of the symbol as a whole. no elements or components or the composition of the symbols were explained to the subjects. experimental group 2 (subjects 2,6,7) was trained by means of an analytic approach. all the symbols were thus discussed in terms of their composition, elements and components. training of the symbols for both groups included three steps (identification, association and labelling) which can also be referred to as one presentation. the association and labelling steps were exactly the same for both groups. during step 2 (association) subjects had to match the symbol to the storyboard picture and/or to the bliss-man (association within a context). step 3, labelling, was done once within the group and then individually, as each individual subject in the group was asked to label the symbol once. the identification process, however, differed for the analytic and the global groups. the identification process for the global group entailed an outline of the visual agreement between the object/picture and the symbol. the symbol together with the picture/object was held up, the visual agreement was pointed out (only for single configuration symbols) and the symbol labelled. during the identification step for group 2, the symbol was shown to the subjects and labelled. the visual agreement between the real object/picture was pointed out and the different symbol elements were analysed. the meaning of each element was explained and visual agreement of elements was pointed out, for example, "to spit", contains three elements (mouth, arrow, action indicator), and all three had to be discussed individually. elements were then synthesized to form a unit again. subjects of this group were thus guided in their analysis and synthesis of symbols during training and the rationale behind each symbol was explained. individual evaluations of each subject's performance on the eight (compound) or seven (single configuration) symbols were performed daily throughout the training period. the aim of this evaluation was to determine progress and to determine when the criterion (100% correct identification of symbols) was met. directly after the training (in other words, as soon as all the candidates in the group had met the criterion of 100% correct identification of symbols), the generalization procedure was administered. the objective of this evaluation was to determine whether the subjects were able to generalize information they had already been taught to new symbols. a non-verbal response (pointing) was thus required for this section of the evaluation. a choice of one out of three unfamiliar symbols that have visual correspondence (a similar semantic element) had to be made (one stimulus item and two distractors). (please refer to appendix c for symbols.) one month after the generalization procedure, subjects' ability to recall the fifteen symbols taught during training was assessed. a one month time period was selected to coincide with the school holidays. during the four week holiday, subjects had no exposure to blissymbols. the same procedure as that used during daily evaluations was followed in this re-evaluation. table 4. weekly outline of the data collection procedures weeks 1 and 2 week 3 week 4, day 1 rest of week 4, week 5 and 6 (12 days of training) day 13 one month following the generalization procedure pre-evaluation pilot study 1 concept and baseline evaluations training of compound symbols: 10 days training of single configuration symbols: 2 days administering of generalization procedure administering of re-evaluation procedure die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 enid moolman & erna alant response definitions a verbal response was expected during the daily evaluations and the re-evaluation procedure in order to eliminate guessing, but a non-verbal response (pointing) during the generalization procedure. responses were scored as correct ( · ) or incorrect (/), and the subject's incorrect responses were also recorded during training, the re-evaluation and generalization procedures. a response was regarded to be incorrect if the subject did not know the answer or gave the wrong answer. symbols that were identified incorrectly were repeated once at the end of the evaluation session to provide the subjects with another opportunity to respond (in both daily evaluations and the reevaluation procedure). data analysis at the beginning of each training session, the exact time was recorded in order to ensure that sessions did not exceed 30 minutes. directly after the training session, the researcher recorded the length of time that the session had lasted (in minutes) as well as the number of presentations for that particular session. "number of presentations" refers to the number of times that the individual symbols were identified, associated with the pictures and labelled by the researcher and/or subjects. "minutes of training" refers to the total number of minutes that were required in order to reach the criterion of 100% correct identification of all the symbols. subjects were then evaluated individually. responses were recorded verbatim and responses were scored as either correct or incorrect. a raw score was therefore obtained. a percentage was calculated in order to determine whether the criterion had been met. this procedure was followed for each training session until the criterion was met. after training, the number of presentations and number of minutes that each subject required to fulfil the criterion for each stage (first the compound symbols and then the single configuration symbols) were calculated. a mean percentage for the global and analytic groups was then calculated for each stage. for the generalization and re-evaluation procedures, responses were once again scored as either correct or incorrect and the subject's responses were written down. following the calculation of a raw score, a percentage for each individual subject was once again calculated, as was a mean percentage for the global and analytic groups. results obtained were firstly analysed in terms of a comparison between the global and analytic groups. a qualitative error analysis was then performed in order to determine the nature of the subjects' errors and difficulties. results a comparison of the results of the global training method group and the analytic training method group the results obtained by group 1 and 2 during training are presented graphically in figures 1 and 2. the results shown in figures 1 and 2 clearly indicate that group 1 required fewer minutes of training to fulfil the criterion for both the single configuration and compound symbols. group 1 required 95 minutes to learn all 15 symbols, whereas group 2 required 155 minutes. thus there is a difference of 60 minutes. although group 2 resingle configuration compound total i global group 3 analytic group total: minutes (mining for compound and single configuration symbols single configuration compound i global group i analytic g r o u p ' total: number of presentations for compound and single configuration symbols figure 1: results obtained during training: length of figure 2: results obtained during the generalization training procedure table 5. results obtained during the generalization procedure group 1 (subjects trained through global training approach) group 2 (subjects trained through analytic training approach) raw scores percentages raw scores percentages individual raw scores 6,6,6 7,6,7 mean scores and percentages 18/30 60% 20/30 66,7% the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) teaching of blissymbols as a bridge into literacy for children with cognitive impairments 79 uired more minutes to fulfil the criterion, the number of q r e s e n t a t i o n s for both groups to learn the compound symbols was the same (9 presentations). group 1, on the other hand, required fewer presentations for the single configuration symbols than group 2 (2 vs 4 presentations). results obtained during the generalization procedure are presented in table 5. in ikble 5 it is interesting to note that group 2 performed slightly better in the generalization procedure, with a mean of 66,7% (20/30), compared to group l's 60% (18/ 30). all the subjects in group 1 obtained a raw score of 6/ 10 whereas two subjects from group 2 scored 7/10 and the third subject scored 6/10. results obtained during the re-evaluation procedure are presented in table 6. as table 6 indicates, group 2 also performed slightly better than group 1 on the re-evaluation procedure. group 2 obtained a mean of 97,8% (44/45), whereas group 1 remembered 91,1% (41/45) of the symbols correctly. three subjects, two from group 2 and one from group 1, remembered all the symbols and scored 100%. error analysis a qualitative error analysis has been performed in order to determine the nature of the subjects' substitutions and difficulties in the three phases (training, re-evaluation and generalization procedures). ease of acquisition was one of the areas of interest. it was scored in terms of number of presentations. during the training phase some symbols were acquired with greater ease than other symbols and differences in the performances of the two groups in terms of ease of acquisition were noted. the results are outlined in table 7. table 6. results obtained during the re-evaluation procedure group 1 (subjects trained through global training approach) group 2 (subjects trained through analytic training approach) raw scores percentages raw scores percentages individual raw scores 13,15,13 15,14,15 mean scores and percentages 41/45 91.1% 44/45 97,8% table 7. an error analysis of the results obtained during the training phase group 2 (analytic training method) group 1 (global training method) symbol mean number of presentations symbol mean number of presentations compound symbols symbols most ' difficult to acquire to smell 5 to fly 6 symbols most ' difficult to acquire rain 5 symbols easiest to | acquire ] i 1 to dance 1.6 to dance 2 symbols easiest to | acquire ] i 1 bath 1.6 bath 2.6 symbols easiest to | acquire ] i 1 to fly 1.6 / single configuration symbols symbols easiest to acquire house 2 house 2 symbols easiest to acquire ear 2 ear 2 symbols easiest to acquire wheel 2 wheel 2 symbols easiest to acquire cloth 2 cloth 2 symbols easiest to acquire table 2 table 2 symbols easiest to acquire eye 2 eye 2 symbols easiest to acquire 2 room 2 symbols most j difficult to acquire room 2.7 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 enid moolman & erna alant group 2 experienced most difficulty with the symbols "to smell" and "rain". a mean of 5 presentations was required to fulfil the criterion. group 2 acquired the symbol "to fly" with great ease, requiring a mean of 1.6 presentations, whereas group 1, by contrast, experienced the greatest difficulty with this symbol, requiring a mean of 6 presentations. similarities were, however, also noted. the symbols "bath", "to dance" and "to fly" were acquired first by group 2 with a mean of 1.6 presentations. group 1 also acquired the symbol "to dance" first, with a mean of 2 presentations, followed by "bath" (mean of 2.6 presentations). the symbols "happy", "to smell" and "rain" were acquired only in the later stages of training in both groups. all the single configuration symbols were acquired with the same degree of ease by group 1, requiring a mean of 2 presentations. one subject from group 2, however, experienced difficulty with the acquisition of the symbol "room" and therefore a mean of 2.7 presentations were required by group 2 to fulfil the criterion. a qualitative analysis of subjects' responses during the generalization procedure revealed a number of interesting points. the results are outlined in table 8. group 2 scored the lowest (0%) for the symbols "to cry" and "sad" and scored 100 % for the symbols "to hear", "bathroom", "garage", "aeroplane" and "low". 66,7% of the group was able to recognise "table cloth" and "legs" and 33,3% recognised "to jump". group 1 scored the lowest for the symbol "sad" (0%) and the highest for the symbol "to hear" (100%). only 33,3% were able to identify "to jump" and "legs", and 66,7% were able to identify the rest of the symbols ["garage", "bathroom", "low", "to cry", "aeroplane", "to dance"]. similarities and differences were once again noted in the performances of the two groups during the generalization procedure. both groups scored 100% for the symbol "to hear" and scored 0% for the symbol "sad". the symbols "bathroom", "garage" and "aeroplane" were identified correctly by most of the subjects of both groups, as group 2 scored 100% and group 1 66,7% correct identification. table 9 provides the results of the re-evaluation procedure for the different word classes as well as the different symbols. during the re-evaluation procedure, group 2 scored a mean of 96,3% for nouns, as opposed to 100% for verbs and adjectives. group 1 remembered 88,9% of the nouns, 100% of the adjectives and 91,7% of the verbs. group 2 thus performed better than group 1 for nouns as well as for verbs. group 2 thus remembered 100% of the compound symbols and 85,7% (6/7) of the single configuration symbols. during the re-evaluation of group 2, only subject 6 responded incorrectly once, as he did not remember the noun "cloth" (single configuration symbol). subjects from group 1 responded incorrectly to the symbols representing the following nouns: "room", "wheel" (single configuration symbols), "rain" (compound symbol) and also to the verb "to spit" (compound symbol). group 1 remembered 71,4% (5/7) of the single configuration and 75% (6/8) of the compound symbols. the qualitative error analysis furthermore revealed that the subjects of both groups remembered more compound than single configuration symbols after a period without any exposure to the symbols. symbol reduction table 8. an error analysis of the results obtained during the generalization procedure % correct (from lowest to highest score) score) group 2 (analytic training method) group 1 (global training method) lowest score: 0% sad sad lowest score: 0% to cry 33,3% to jump to jump 33,3% legs 66,7% table cloth garage 66,7% legs bathroom ! 66,7% low 1 66,7% to cry 66,7% aeroplane 66,7% to dance highest score: 100% to hear to hear highest score: 100% bathroom / highest score: 100% garage highest score: 100% aeroplane highest score: 100% low the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) teaching of blissymbols as a bridge into literacy for children with cognitive impairments table 9. an error analysis of the results obtained during the re-evaluation procedure: word classes and symbols % correct group 2 (analytic training method) group 1 (global training method) word classes nouns 96,3 88,9 word classes verbs 100 91,7 word classes adjectives 100 100 symbols single configuration symbols 85,7 71,4 symbols compound symbols 100 75 was noted within group 2 during training, as subjects often only remembered one element and responded accordingly. indicators were also often ignored by this group. during the generalization procedure, the subjects of both groups scored 100% correct identification for the symbol "to hear", and 0% correct identification for the symbol "sad". group 2 also scored 0% for the symbol "to cry". discussion from the above results it is clear that group 2 required much more time (in minutes) during the training-phase to reach the criterion of 100%. the main reason for this is most probably the nature of the teaching strategy. the teaching strategy used involves three steps, namely identification, association and labelling. these three steps can also be referred to as "one presentation". only the identification process differs for ithe two groups and led to the longer training period for group 2. the length of training obviously has important implications, particularly for educators and clinicians. time is a very important factor in the lives of the handicapped. alot of time goes into therapy and very little time is left [over for educational programs (light & kelford smith, 1993; smith, 1992). the implication is therefore that clinicians/educators often choose the approach which takes leastjtime, as opposed to an approach which takes much longer,! but gives the same initial results. this is supported by literature on the teaching of reading (greyling & joubert, 1989). colheart (in blau, 1988) suggested that the two routes to reading operate parallel to each other, but that the direct visual route (global approach) is a faster method for lexical access than the indirect visual route (analytic approach). many teachers therefore prefer a global approach to an analytic approach due to the shorter teaching time. however, the hypothesis can be argued that the time required for training (teaching strategy) is reduced the longer the subjects are exposed to blissymbols. the reason for this is that more symbols and elements are already known and therefore require less/no explanation, as well as the fact that subjects might be more familiar with the processes of analysis and synthesis. light and lindsay (1991) refer to this as "automaticity". according to these researchers the cognitive operations become fully automatized with repeated practise, and as a result they no longer require attentional resources and hence do not use up any of the limited space in working memory. what is clear, however, is that a criterion of 100% correct identification of symbols does not necessarily imply that the skills of analysis and synthesis are established. educators need to help cognitively impaired children more (and in other ways) with the transfer of these skills. one possibility could be to combine symbol elements with a variety of other symbol elements in as many contexts as possible, for example, "house" + "wheel"; "house" + "animal"; "house" + "envelope", etc. in other words, the number of different recurrent elements should be increased in a variety of contexts. the finding that group 2 required more time to reach the criterion does not necessarily imply that these subjects experienced more difficulty in acquiring the symbols. the ease of acquisition (scored in terms of number of presentations) for compound symbols was the same for both groups. in terms of ease of acquisition of compound symbols, the one method therefore did not seem to be more effective than the other. in fact, shepherd and haaf (1992) found that their non-disabled subjects trained using the composite meaning method (the analytic approach) required fewer trials for learning than those subjects trained via whole symbol memorization (the global approach). it therefore appeared to be easier for subjects to learn blissymbols when trained to their component elements. one must bear in mind, however, that results obtained by non-disabled individuals cannot be applied uncritically to cognitively impaired individuals. it is worth noting that group 1 in this study required a smaller number of presentations to reach the criterion for the single configuration symbols (2 vs 4 presentations), but this observation alone was not the main reason for the noticeable difference in the length of training required by the two groups. one has to conclude that the methodology or teaching strategy of the analytic approach is the main factor/cause for the longer training period required. it is important to note that the subjects of group 2 initially (day 1 of training) seemed overwhelmed by the different components/elements and therefore scored less well than group 1. on day 2, however, they scored much better and the difference between the two groups' performances was much smaller. this might be explained by the complexity of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 enid moolman & erna alant blissymbols. according to luftig and bersani (1985), it appears that component complexity may initially confuse a naive learner as he/she struggles to learn a new list of unfamiliar symbols. these researchers, however, mentioned that the effects of component complexity are reduced as the learner becomes more familiar and has to link the symbols with a new set of responses. according to luftig and bersani (1985), clinicians should introduce the less complex symbols first, until the learner is familiar with the nature of the system. in this study, however, compound symbols were introduced first in order to differentiate clearly between the two stages and the two distinct training strategies. it was felt that training single configuration symbols first would influence the subjects of group 2's orientation to an analytic approach. at this stage it is uncertain whether this aspect has influenced the results of the study or not, as the study had no control group. it is, however, clear that all the subjects, particularly those of group 1, acquired the single configuration symbols with real ease, requiring only 2 presentations to fulfil the criterion (4 presentations for group 2). an important difference was noted in the performance of the two groups during the re-evaluation and generalization procedures. the results of this study, as well as of that of shepherd and haaf (1992), suggest that subjects trained by means of an analytic approach benefitted more in the long term than those trained by means of a global approach. the results of this study indicate that there are no major differences between the two groups' performances, but that group 2 consistently tended to perform better on the re-evaluation and generalization procedures. consequently, this phenomenon suggests that the analytic approach has long-term benefits. in the selection of a teaching approach for the teaching of blissymbols, the long-term effects must definitely be considered. group 2 performed slightly better than group 1 on the re-evaluation procedure. whatever advantages were gained in the analytic training approach were maintained over the one month period without training. this confirms shepherd and haaf's (1992) research, as they also found that their nondisabled subjects trained by composite meaning (analytically) out-performed subjects trained via whole symbol memorization (global approach) in the re-evaluation phase. this raises the question as to why group 2 remembered the symbols better, but did not necessarily learn them faster than group 1. schlosser and lloyd (1992) postulated that it is possible that acquisition and retention require different skills. "hypothetically speaking, acquisition testing may have been mastered primarily through immediate recall, while retention testing may have required more reliance on an analysis of the cues provided (compound blissymbols) on the testboards" (schlosser & lloyd, 1992:25). an analytic training approach may have facilitated the analysis of these cues as well as aided recognition memory. if a child only remembers the element" bread" in the compound "toast", for example, he might be able to extrapolate the other elements and the combined meaning or make a good guess (schlosser & lloyd, 1992). an alternative explanation for group 2's better performance on the re-evaluation procedure might be that the subjects understood the logic behind the symbols, and because they knew the different elements, it was easier for them to remember the symbols. for group 1, it was merely a visual symbol that they had to memorize, with no logic behind the symbols. that could also explain why subjects of group 1 substituted symbols such as "rain", with "to fly" and "to smell", and the symbol "to spit", with "to dance" and "flower". there was no correlation between the actual symbol and the substitute symbols (in terms of elements) it seemed as if they rather relied on guessing and thus showed limited coping skills. results also indicate that the subjects from group 2 did slightly better on the generalization procedure. this also confirms shepherd and haaf's (1992) study. they found that subjects who were trained via the composite meaning method (analytic approach) were able to generalize symbol knowledge more effectively than subjects trained via whole symbol memorization (global approach). johnsen and jennische (in mcnaughton, 1993) suggested that the processing skills of segmenting and sequencing that a child develops by using type 2 symbols (such as sequenced ideographic blissymbols) can help prepare the child for the processing of print. an analytic training approach therefore equips the child with the potential to transfer these skills to novel symbols. the difference between the two groups' performances was small, and thus statistically insignificant, and this raises the question of why the differences were not more marked. one factor could be that the skills of analysis and synthesis were not totally established and carried over. although some of the subjects knew all the different components of each of the training symbols, they were not ready to transfer these skills to novel symbols. they were, however, able to do so in most of the cases and were well on their way to achieving these skills. a longer training period would probably have helped the individuals with the transfer of these skills. a qualitative error analysis led to a few interesting observations with regard to the acquisition, retention and generalization of specific symbols. these are discussed briefly. according to many researchers (bloomberg, karlan & lloyd, 1990; blackstone, 1990), translucency seems to have a positive effect on learning when individuals are learning to pair an already familiar spoken word with a symbol. iconic symbols are easier to learn because most iconic symbols represent concrete objects/entities (blackstone, 1990). it is therefore not surprising that the single, configuration symbols were learned by both groups after a smaller number of presentations than the compound symbols. however, single configuration symbols were not retained better than the compound symbols by either group. with the re-evaluation procedure, group 2 only had one symbol wrong, namely "cloth" (doek). group 1 also remembered more compound symbols than single configurations. although a number of reasons are possible, the two main reasons that contributed to this phenomenon would appear to be the level of possible visual discrimination and component complexity. "visual discriminability" (in other words, when a group of related symbols appear very similar with only minor differences) could have played a role in the results that were obtained. musselwhite and ruscello (1984) reported that the "discriminability" of the symbols can pose a possible identification (and, it seems in this case, retention) problem. in an informal analysis, schlosser and lloyd (1992) found that the number of elements in compounds does not correlate with the degree of acquisition when elements are pretaught (analytic approach). however, the number of the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) teaching of blissymbols as a bridge into literacy for children with cognitive impairments elements in compounds seems to correlate negatively with the degree of acquisition when compounds are taught directly (global approach). in other words, the fewer elements in a compound, the better the degree of acquisition if a global training approach is used (schlosser & lloyd, 1992). this could explain why group 1 required only 2 presentations in the acquisition of single configuration symbols, versus group 2's 4 presentations. l b summarize: no major differences between the two training groups' performances were noted, but there was a definite tendency for group 2 to perform better on the re-evaluation and generalization procedures. conclusions the purpose of the study was to compare two training approaches (global vs analytic) in the learning of blissymbols by mildly cognitively impaired subjects. the following results were obtained: a slight difference between the two groups' performances in all the areas was noted. group 2 consistently tended to perform better in the reevaluation and generalization procedures. one could hypothesize that if the training period were to be continued, even stronger gains could have been made by group 2. the analytic training approach, however, was much more time-consuming than the global approach. the two groups required almost the same number of presentations to fulfil the criterion (with group 1 requiring two presentations less than group 2) and therefore one can conclude that the ease of acquisition was similar for both groups. a qualitative analysis reveals that compound symbols are retained better over a one month period without any exposure than single configuration symbols are. individual variations were noted throughout training. subjects experienced specific problems with the generalization of some of the symbols, such as "to cry" and "sad", but then again acquired other symbols such as "bath" / "to dance" with relative ease. one can thus conclude that mildly cognitively impaired subjects are able to learn blissymbols by means of either an analytic approach or a global approach, but that individuals seemed to benefit more from an analytic approach. more emphasis should, however, be placed on the teaching of indicators land abstract elements. whether or not these conclusions extend to individuals with more severe levels of cognitive impairment is subject to further inquiry. ; this study confirmed .that blissymbols can be taught successfully to the mildly cognitively impaired child through either a global and/or an analytic approach. although there was only a slight difference between the two groups' performances during training, there was a consistent tendency for group 2 to perform better than group 1 in the re-evaluation and generalization procedures. this study thus provides initial data to suggest that the training of blissymbols through an analytic approach provides advantages in the retention of symbols as well as with regard to the generalization to new symbols. as cognitively impaired individuals may experience problems with analysis and synthesis, the use of an analytic approach to help such children to cope with these skills in relation to normal orthography is important. also, if one considers the benefits that an analytic approach offers, in that it prepares the child to cope with the processes of analysis and synthesis which are required to learn normal orthography by means of a phonetic approach, the analytic approach seems to be preferable. one should, however, not see blissymbol teaching as an either/or approach, but one should rather try to balance longand short-term goals and teach whatever is needed (whether this is pictographic/single configuration or compound symbols) (schlosser & lloyd, 1992). this recommendation is also in line with the suggestion made by ehri (in gough, ehri & treiman, 1992) to use a combined approach rather than an either/or approach in teaching reading. references alant, e. (1994). the use of blissymbols as a first step into literacy with four children with down syndrome. the south african journal of communication disorders, 41, 23-32. archer, l.a. (1977). blissymbolics a nonverbal communication system. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 42, 568579. beery, k.e. (1967). developmental test for visual-motor integration. cleveland: d.h. modern curriculum. blackstone, s.w. (ed.) (1990). graphic symbols for communication, language and learning. augmentative communication news, 3, 1-3. blau, a.f. (1988). fostering literacy development. in: blackstone, s.w., cassata-james, e. & bruskin, d.m. (eds), augmentative communication: implementation strategies. maryland: asha. bloomberg, k , karlan, g.r. & lloyd, l.l. (1990). the comparative translucency of initial lexical items represented in five graphic symbol systems and sets. journal of speech and hearing research, 33, 717-725. buttery, t.j. & creekmore, w.n. (1985). planning reading instruction for the mildly handicapped child. reading improvement, 206-212. folk, m.c. & campbell, j. (1978). teaching functional reading to the tmr. education and training of the mentally retarded, 13, 322-326. frostig, m.f. (1963). developmental test of visual perception. california: palo alto. fuller, d.r. (1992). the effects of translucency, complexity, and response mode on the learning of blissymbols by preschool children and adults with normal cognitive abilities. paper presented at isaac conference, philadelphia. goodenough, f.l. (1926). the measurement of intelligence by drawings. london: harrap. gough, p.b., ehri, l.c. & treiman, r. (1992). reading acquisition. new jersey: lawrence erlbaum associates. greyling, p.j. & joubert, j.j. (1989). didactics: reading instruction in the junior primary phase. pretoria: de jagerhaum publishers. hehner, b. (1980). blissymbols for use. toronto: blissymbolics communication institute. koppenhaver, d.a. & yoder, d.e. (1990). facilitating literacy learning in children with speech and physical impairments. paper presented at isaac conference, stockholm, sweden. koppenhaver, d.a., coleman, p.p., kalman, s.l. & yoder, d.e. (1991). the implications of emergent literacy research for children with developmental disabilities. american journal of speech-language pathology, 1, 38-44. kritzinger, a. 1985. afrikaans translation of the "test of auditory comprehension of language ii (tacl): original compiler: carrow-woolfolk. texas: dlm teaching resources. light, j. & lindsay, p. (1991). cognitive science and augmentative and alternative communication. augmentative and alternative communication, 7, 186-203. light, j. & kelford smith, a. (1993). home literacy experiences of preschoolers who use aac systems and of their nondisabled peers. augmentative and alternative communication, 9, 1025. luftig, r.l. & bersani, h.a. (jr). (1985). an investigation of two variables influencing blissymbol learnability with nonhandicapped adults. augmentative alternative communication, 1, 32-37. mcdonald, e.t. (1989). teaching and using blissymbolics. toronto: blissymbolics communication institute. mcnaughton, s. (1993). graphic representational systems and literacy learning. topics in language disorders, 13, 58-75. miller, l. (1990). the roles of language and learning in the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 84 development of literacy. topics in language disorders, 10, 124. moolman, e. (1994). a comparison of two training approaches in the learning of blissymbols by cognitively handicapped children. unpublished m-logopaedics thesis, university of pretoria. musselwhite, c.r. & ruscello, d.m. (1984). transparency of three communication symbol systems. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 27, 436-443. raver, s.a. & dwyer, r.c. (1986). teaching handicapped preschoolers to sight read using language training procedures. the reading teacher, 40, 314-321. romski, m.a., sevcik, r., pate, j. & rumbaugh, d. (1985). discrimination of lexigrams and traditional orthography by nonspeaking severely mentally retarded persons. american journal of mental deficiency, 90, 185-189. e n i d m o o l m a n & e r n a alant schlosser, r.w. & lloyd, l.l. (1992). effects of initial element teaching in a story-telling context on blissymbol acquisition and generalization. purdue university, west lafayette, in. shepherd, t.a. & haaf, r. (1992). a comparison of two training methods of blis-symbols. paper presented at isaac conference, philadelphia. singh, n.n. & singh, j. (1986). reading acquisition and remediation in the mentally retarded. international review of research in mental retardation, 14, 165-199. smith, m.m. (1992). literacy and aac: methodological issues and research priorities. paper presented at isaac conference, philadelphia. sterick, g. (1979). a follow-up study often children who learned to read in a class for trainable students. education and training of the mentally retarded, 14, 170-176. a p p e n d i x a a p p e n d i x β graphic r e p r e s e n t a t i o n of s y m b o l s u s e d d u r i n g t r a i n i n g a n d g e n e r a l i z a t i o n proc e d u r e s c o m p o u n d s y m b o l s u s e d d u r i n g training (stage 1) single c o n f i g u r a t i o n s y m b o l s u s e d d u r ing training (stage 2) s y m b o l g r a p h i c r e p r e s e n t a t i o n b a d (bath) r e e n (rain) ψ spoeg (to spit) έν vlieg (to fly) dans (to d a n c e ) ruik (to s m e l l ) 1 bly / g e l u k k i g (happy) c n h o o g ( h i g h ) s y m b o l g r a p h i c r e p r e s e n t a t i o n tafel (table) 1—. d o e k (cloth) η huis (house) ώ w i e l (wheel) β o o g (eye) ο k a m e r (room) η o o r (ear) ! ι the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t b t e a c h i n g of blissymbols as a bridge into literacy for children with cognitive impairments 85 appendix c symbols used during the generalization procedure g^lmulus item additional symbol additional symbol tafeldoek (table cloth) # 1 — 1 mat (carpet) ή ι ζ ] servet (napkin) # 2 motorhuis (garage) poskantoor (post office) 0 0 skuur (barn) huil (to cry) j\ ο φ slaap (to sleep) a m reenjas (raincoat) badkamer (bathroom) klaskamer (classroom) z l v l / o eetkamer (diningroom) ί ο hoor (to hear) a ο asem (breath) a. z o bril (classes) e n o a spring (to jump) λ. swaai (to swing) λ meng (to mix) vliegtuig (aeroplane) ύ wa (wagon) rolskaatse (roller skates) a ^ si bene/voete (legs/feet) klim (to climb) z l t kom (to come) λ ongelukkig (sad) 0 j , troos (to comfort) φ ώ vrede (peace) ώ laag (low) i ! < dun (thin) v ί κ groot (big) v i appendix d results of the pre-evaluation test subjects j 2 3 4 5 6 7 area assessed: } ae ae ae ae ae ae a. receptive language: (months) 45-48 word classes and relations: 48-51 47-50 51-54 42-44 59-65 45-48 grammatical morphemes: 49-53 58-63 58-63 54-58 46-50 59-65 elaborated sentences: 61-64 71-75 42-45 42-45 71-75 40-43 b. auditory perception: <6,0 auditory memory 6,8-6,11 <6,0 8,0-8,3 <6,0 7,4-7,7 <6,0 auditory sequencing 6,0-6,3 6,0-6,3 6,0-6,3 6,8-6,11 6,4-6,7 <6,0 auditory analysis <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 (pendulum score) subjective evaluation: 5/5 *2 syllable words: 5/5 5/5 5/5 5/5 5/5 5/5 *3 syllable words: 3/5 3/5 0/5 3/5 5/5 5/5 *4 syllable words: 1/5 0/5 0/5 1/5 0/5 0/5 auditory closure <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 <6,8<6,0 <6,0 suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 86 enid moolman & erna alant auditory synthesis <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 <6,0 sound blending 7,7 5,3 7,1 6,2 6,8 5,3 c. reading: names of classmates: % recognized % labelled 100 71 100 71 93 79 46 18 27 18 100 100 functional words: % recognized % labelled 100 100 67 44 100 75 38 0 38 25 78 56 sight words: % recognized % labelled 67 75 100 83 67 33 0 0 0 0 67 50 average score: % word recognition 89 89 87 28 22 2 average score: % word labelling 82 66 63 6 14 69 d. expressive language: mean length of utterance 6,6 2,3 5,9 5,30 6,1 4,1 predicted chronological age (months) >58,3 29,3 57,5 52,8 >58,3 43,4 speech intelligibility (see scale) 5 3 5 5 3 5 e. visual perception rating: average score (the average score includes all the subtests provided below) good 4,8 good 4,4 average good 4,4 average 4,5 average 4,3 good 4,9 *key: ae: age equivalent ps: although the test of oral language production was administered, the results are not provided here, as it was thought to be unreliable. subjects experienced extreme difficulty with story formulation, and were unable to provide a meaningful story most of the time. ( results of the visual perception testing subtests s 2 s4 s7 s3 s s 5 , draw-a-man test 3,6 3,9 4,6 4,3 3,6 4,0 j visual-motor integration 3,9 3,6 4,9 4,4 4,9 4,9 ; hand-eye co-ordination 4,0 3,9 4,9 4,9 4,6 4,6 ' foreground-background 3,3 3,6 3,6 3,9 4 3,9 form consistency 5,6 6,0 5,6 4,6 4 4,6 position in space 5,0 5,0 5,0 4,9 4,9 5,6 ' spatial relationships 4,9 3,0 4,9 5 4,9 4,9 . memory (subjective evaluation) 8 6,0 6,0 4 4 ' 5 sequencing (subjective evaluation) 5 4,6 4,6 4 4 4 total score 43,3 39,6 44,1 40 38,9 40,7 average score 4,8 4,4 4,9 4,4 4,3 4,5 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 49 the knowledge and attitudes of occupational therapy, physiotherapy and speech-language therapy students, regarding the speech-language therapist's role in the hospital stroke rehabilitation team. lisa felsher and eleanor ross department of speech pathology and audiology university of the 'witwatersrand abstract the purpose of the present study was to survey and compare the knowledge and attitudes of final year occupational therapy, physiotherapy and speech-language therapy students, concerning the role of the speech-language therapist as a member of the stroke rehabilitation team in the hospital setting. in order to achieve this aim, a questionnaire was administered to final year students in these three disciplines, and included questions on most areas of stroke rehabilitation with which the speech-language therapist might be involved, as well as the concepts of rehabilitation and teamwork in relation to stroke rehabilitation. results suggested a fairly good understanding of the concepts of rehabilitation and teamwork. students appeared to have a greater understanding of those disorders following a stroke, with which the speech-language therapist is commonly involved, such as aphasia, dysarthria, verbal apraxia and dysphagia. however, students appeared to show less understanding of those disorders post-stroke, for which the speechlanguage therapist's role is less well defined, such as agraphia, alexia and amnesia. in addition, a high percentage of role duplication / overlapping in several aspects of stroke rehabilitation, such as family and social support, was found. several implications for facilitating communication, collaboration and understanding between paramedical professions, as well as for further research are also provided. opsomming die doel van die huidige studie was om die kennis en houdings van finalejaarstudente in arbeids-, fisio en spraaktaalterapie ten opsigte vdn die rol van die spraak-taalterapeut as lid van die beroerterehabilitasiespan in die hospitaalopset te ondersoek en te vergelyk. om die doel te bereik, is 'n vraelys deur die finalejaarstudente in die drie dissiplines voltooi en is vrae in die meeste areas van beroerterehabilitasie waarby die spraak-taalterapeut betrokke mag wees, sowel as konsepte van rehabilitasie en spanwerk in verband met beroerterehabilitasie, ingesluit. die resultate^ dui op 'n redelike goeie begrip van die konsepte van spanwerk en rehabilitasie. die studente blyk 'n beter begrip te he vir die afwykings na 'n beroerte waarby die spraak-taalterapeut dikwels betrokke is, soos afasie, disartrie, verbale apraksie en disfagie. die studente toon egter minder begrip van die afwykings na beroerte waar die spraak-taalterapeut se rol minder duidelik gedefinieer is soos agrafie, aleksie en amnesie. verder is 'n hoe persentasie van die funksies dupliserend of oorvleuelend bevind in 'n aantal aspekte van beroerterehabilitasie soos familiale en sosiale ondersteuning. verskeie implikasies vir die fasilitering van kommunikasie en begrip tussen paramediese professionele personeel, sowel as vir verdere navorsing is ook verskaf. stroke is r e g a r d e d as o n e of the l e a d i n g c a u s e s of d e a t h and disability in all races in south africa (fritz & penn, 1992). t h e t e r m " s t r o k e " is s y n o n y m o u s w i t h c e r e b r o v a s c u l a r accident (cva), which a c c o r d i n g to the world h e a l t h o r g a n i s a t i o n ( w h o ) , can be defined as "a sudden ( a c u t e ) d i s t u r b a n c e ) of b r a i n (cerebral) function of v a s c u l a r origin, c a u s i n g disability lasting m o r e t h a n , or d e a t h w i t h i n , 24 h o u r s " ( c l i f f o r d r o s e & c a p i l d e o , 1981). t h e c o n s e q u e n c e s of s t r o k e are far r a n g i n g and i n c l u d e p h y s i c a l , c o m m u n i c a t i o n , social, p e r s o n a l and e c o n o m i c d i s t u r b a n c e s . a p a r t from the m e d i c a l attention stroke patients m a y need, they m a y also require n u r s i n g s u p p o r t , and will usually need help in r e c o v e r i n g their physical, e m o t i o n a l and social functioning (wade, langton-hewer, skilbeck & david, 1985). thus, m o r e often the major thrust of t h e r a p y is in t e a c h i n g the patient to live in a n o r m a l e n v i r o n m e n t , providing family support, social welfare a s s i s t a n c e , p s y c h o logical care, and rehabilitation of speech, m o v e m e n t and co-ordination (fritz & penn, 1992). c h a m b e r l a i n (1989, p.311) defined r e h a b i l i t a t i o n as "a process designed to m a x i m i s e a patient's physical, m e n t a l , social and v o c a t i o n a l p o t e n t i a l . " a s no single health professional can be expert in all t h e s e areas, a t e a m of professionals is necessary for a r e h a b i l i t a t i v e p r o g r a m m e to b e effective ( s t o n n i n g t o n & b r o w n e , die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 lisa felsher & eleanor ross 1987). dean & geiringer (1990, p.275) add that the ideal rehabilitation team uses an interdisciplinary approach, whereby "individual professionals from different disciplines evaluate patients according to their own speciality and contribute to a team conference for the patient." through the synchronous endeavours of multiple specialists, rehabilitation thus attempts to identify all disturbances and their causes, attempting to reduce them, while s i m u l t a n e o u s l y identifying c o m p e n s a t i n g strengths (bluestone, 1976). the concept of team management is rapidly becoming accepted as one of the most holistic, economic and efficient means of providing a patient with the best possible care (haller & sheldon, 1976; boone, 1987). while many advantages of teamwork have been listed in the literature, such as the experience in participatory learning for team members and the provision of a comprehensive but integrated range of services for the patient (kane, 1975; germain, 1984; stonnington & browne, 1987), several disadvantages to team practice also exist. these include a lack of knowledge abovit the education and expertise of other disciplines, the problem of role definition that exists among team members, and the fostering of undesirable competition or professional jealousy. according to penn (1992), a large proportion of people who have had a stroke, suffer from a communication disability. hence, the speech-language therapist who specializes in communication disorders, is an integral member of the professional partnership that exists between rehabilitation specialists. "communication is a basic function, taken for granted until it fails" (wade et al., 1985, p. 135). the three most common disorders of communication resulting from a stroke which affect speech and language are aphasia, dysarthria and apraxia. aphasia refers to "the loss or impairment of the ability to use and understand language" (fritz & penn, 1992, p.203), while dysarthria is defined as an "impairment in articulation of sounds caused by muscle weaknesses and incoordination of muscles responsible for speech and breathing" (fritz & penn, 1992, p.205). in contrast, apraxia, which is "a disorder of learned movement distinct from paralysis, weakness and incoordination, that results in a disturbance in motor-planning" (love & webb, 1986, p. 275). in addition, dysphagia which is a disorder of swallowing, occurs in approximately one third of stroke patients (penn, 1992). communication disorders "often impinge upon social, emotional and financial well-being" (simmons, 1985, p.7). as a result, "speech pathology and audiology have a closer affinity to rehabilitation medicine than any other department in a hospital" (bluestone, 1976, p. 12). moreover, simmons (1981) states that the speech-language therapist, who is experienced in the diagnosis and management of neuropathologies of communication, can be a major contributor to the care of the neurologically impaired patient. according to softley (1987), the aims of treating speech and language disorders in the stroke patient, are the restoration of an effective communication system and the readjustment of the patient and his/her family, to changes in the way they communicate. in addition penn (1992, p.55) states that "a speech and language therapist teaches patients to communicate as fully as possible, assessing and treating problems of speech, hearing, understanding, verbal and gestural expression, and reading and writing disorders." according to simmons (1981), the management of the patient with a neurogenic communication disorder ideally involves a co-operative interaction between medical specialists and speech-language therapists. in addition, a variety of other professionals are extremely i m p o r t a n t in the m a n a g e m e n t of p a t i e n t s with neurogenic communication disorders. while physiotherapists and occupational therapists improve the physical foundations for successful communication and contribute information on the integrity of sensori-motor systems, the neuropsychologist where appropriate, administers comprehensive test batteries to provide data on various aspects of intelligence and mental status (simmons, 1981). from the previous discussion, it would appear that the speech-language therapist has a crucial role to play in the stroke rehabilitation team. however, it would seem that there is often a lack of understanding among medical and paramedical personnel regarding the role and functions of speech and language therapy, with the result that the patients requiring such services may not always be referred to a speech-language therapist (clifford-rose & capildeo, 1981). this fact is illustrated by milner (1989), who investigated the knowledge and attitudes of senior medical students concerning the role of the speech-language and hearing therapist in the treatment of communication disorders. she concluded that most members of the medical profession remain unaware of the critical importance of speech and hearing therapy services for individuals whose communication abilities are impaired. findings in this study suggested that the medical students required education concerning the profession of speech and hearing therapy, and "the assistance that speech and hearing therapists can offer as members of the health team" (p.30). furthermore, motona and nowitz (1991) attempted to establish qualified physiotherapists' appropriate definitions of rehabilitation, and their views regarding what it encompasses. included in a questionnaire, were questions pertaining to the composition and leadership of the rehabilitation team. the speech-language therapist was seen to be an essential member of the team, however, the extent to which he/ she is involved in the stroke rehabilitation team was not established. in a study by waller and murphy (1985), speech-language therapists who work in a medical setting, were requested to indicate how well other rehabilitation workers understood the role of the speech-language therapist. approximately sixty percent of the occupational therapists, and approximately fifty percent of the physiotherapists indicated that they had a good understanding of the speech-language therapist's role, while the remaining percentage of each profession had only some or no understanding. as a result of these research findings, it appeared to be both relevant and timely to investigate the knowledge and attitudes of final year occupational therapy and physiotherapy students towards the role of the speech-language therapist as a member, of the stroke rehabilitation team in a hospital setting, and to compare their views with those of final year speech and hearing therapy students. since final year students represent future allied health professionals in the new south africa, it was envisaged that a survey-type rethe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the knowledge and attitudes of occupational therapy, physiotherapy & speech-language therapy students, regarding the speech-language therapist's role in the hospital stroke rehabilitation team 51 s e a r c h project targeted, at final year students, would provide some indication of the preparation they receive during their formal undergraduate training, in terms of stroke rehabilitation and the various professions inv o l v e d in an inter-disciplinary team. furthermore, it was felt that participation in the research study might serve an educational purpose by enhancing respondents' i n s i g h t into, and awareness of, the profession of speechl a n g u a g e pathology in relation to stroke rehabilitation. methodology i. aim of the study 1. to investigate the knowledge and attitudes of final year occupational therapy, physiotherapy and speechlanguage therapy students, regarding the role of the speech-language therapist as a member of the stroke rehabilitation team in a hospital setting. 2. to descriptively compare the views of occupational therapy and physiotherapy students with those of speech-language therapy students. ii. subjects and criteria for selection final year (fourth year) occupational therapy, physiotherapy and speech-language therapy students, registered at the university of the witwatersrand were used as subjects in the study. iii. research design in order to achieve the aims of the study, a crosssectional survey research design was implemented which involved the administration of a questionnaire within a group setting. questionnaire content the questionnaire consisted of 30 questions or items and was divided into four sections. a copy of the questionnaire is set out in appendix a. section a consisted of twojclosed-ended questions that elicited biographical information. section β consisted of open-ended and closed-ended questions that focused on rehabilitation and teamwork. ι section c ' 7 this section consisted of questions that required the subjects to indicate which professional(s) would be responsible for various aspects of the stroke patient's rehabilitation. these aspects included :motor recovery of limbs; the treatment of communication problems; emotional adjustment of the patient; family and social support; cognitive rehabilitation; and the management of long-term occupation. these questions were included to ascertain whether the respondents felt that more than one discipline would be responsible for each aspect of the stroke patient's rehabilitation, i.e., role duplication/ overlapping, and whether each group of students tended to assign most aspects of stroke rehabilitation to their own profession. in addition, the researcher attempted to establish each student group's perception regarding the speech-language therapist's role or responsibility in the above-mentioned aspects of stroke rehabilitation. section d this section consisted of 15 statements encompassing the diversity of disorders which may present post stroke, and with which the speech-language therapist might be involved, such as aphasia, dysarthria, apraxia and dysphagia. iv. procedure a) piloting the questionnaire questionnaires were distributed to a recently qualified speech therapist, an occupational therapist and a physiotherapist, all of whom work with stroke patients in a hospital setting. following the pilot study, certain questions were modified and rephrased. b) administration of the questionnaire sixty-seven questionnaires were distributed to students and collected during the same afternoon by the researcher. v. treatment of data descriptive statistics comprising pie-charts "and tables, with frequencies and percentages were employed. those questionnaire items that were presented in the form of a likert scale, were analyzed according to a three-category system. the five-category system was reduced to a three-category system. therefore, "strongly agree" and "agree" categories were combined, as well as the "strongly disagree" and "disagree" categories. the "undecided" category remained unchanged. open-ended questionnaire items were described both quantitatively and qualitatively. results and discussion (limitation of space precludes the inclusion of figures or tables to illustrate all the questions.) section a question 1a the pie-charts in figure 1 represent the percentage of respondents who indicated that they either had or had not, worked with stroke patients respectively. it is interesting to note that 42,9 % of the speech-language therapy students had not worked with stroke patients. the reason for this may possibly be attributed to the fact that only in their fourth year of study, are speechlanguage therapy students provided with the opportunity to work with stroke patients. this opportunity arises either during a six week hospital block, or at the speech and hearing clinic at the university of the witwatersrand. in comparison, only 23,1 percent of physiotherapy students had no experience working with stroke patients, while all occupational therapy students had worked with the latter. question ib ninety percent of the occupational therapy students, and 57,7% of the physiotherapy students reported that they had worked with stroke patients who had communication problems. this may have influenced the occupational students' understanding of communication disorders post-stroke, as well as the speech-language therapist's role in the stroke patient's rehabilitation. die suid-afrikaanse rif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 lisa felsher & eleanor ross question 2a the physiotherapy and occupational therapy students were requested to indicate whether they had contact with speech-language therapists during their clinical blocks, while the speech-language therapy students were requested to indicate whether they had contact with occupational therapists and/or physiotherapists. findings reflected similar percentages of contact suggesting that all three student groups may have been equally exposed to other rehabilitation specialities. question 2b this question required occupational therapy and physiotherapy students to specify where they had made contact with speech-language therapists, and speech-language therapy students were requested to specify where they had made contact with occupational therapists and/or physiotherapists. twenty physiotherapy students (76,9%) and 15 occupational therapy students (75%) had made contact with speech-language therapists, while 15 speech-language therapy students (71,4%) had made contact with occupational therapists and/or physiotherapists. most contact appeared to occur within the hospital setting, while all three student groups indicated that they had been exposed to other therapists at schools for cerebral palsied children such as forest town school. section β question 1 definition of rehabilitation nichols (1976), suggests that all clinicians should be concerned with the rehabilitation of their patients, but there are many who will not have had the opportunity as students to develop an understanding of the wider aspects of patient care called "rehabilitation." in analyzing the definitions of rehabilitation put forward by the physiotherapy, occupational therapy and speech-language therapy students, the salient points (displayed in table 1 below) of current definitions of rehabilitation by gloag (1985), gresham (1986), galley & forster (1987), purtilo (1988), walker (1988) and chamberlain (1989), were taken into account. table 1 below highlights the percentage of respondents for each group, which included these prominent features in their definitions of rehabilitation. the emphasis of point one in table 1 is that rehabilitation aims at helping the individual reach his/her maximal potential. a relatively lower percentage of speech-language therapy students mentioned this point in their definitions of rehabilitation, while its prominent occurrence in the majority of current definitions in the literature is evident. point two of table 1 highlighted the various aspects with which rehabilitation is concerned. a relatively higher percentage of physiotherapy students included this point in their definitions, which is also found in most definitions in the literature. while some of the physiotherapy and occupational therapy students mentioned the goal of working toward patient independence (point three), none of the speechlanguage therapy students mentioned this factor. it is interesting to note that a very low percentage of respondents across all student groups, mentioned the holistic nature of rehabilitation (point five). this finding may possibly be attributed to the fact that students are not provided with the opportunity to work within an interdisciplinary, holistic environment. this implies that students might not see themselves as members of a larger stroke rehabilitation team, but rather perceive themselves as being the exclusive providers of occupational, speech and physiotherapy respectively. on the whole, the speech-language therapy students provided the narrowest definitions of rehabilitation, suggesting a possible under-emphasis of this concept during their theoretical and clinical training. o t s p t s 100.0% 7 6 . 9 % l h i e x p e r i e n c e w i t h s t r o k e p a t i e n t s n o e x p e r i e n c e o t s = o c c u p a t i o n a l t h e r a p y s t u d e n t s (n-20) p t s = p h y s i o t h e r a p y s t u d e n t s (n-26) s t s = s p e e c h l a n g u a g e t h e r a p y s t u d e n t s (n-21) figure 1. experience with stroke patients s t s 5 7 . 1 % 2 3 . 1 % 4 2 . 9 % the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the knowledge and attitudes of occupational therapy, physiotherapy & speech-language therapy students, regarding the speech-language therapist's role in the hospital stroke rehabilitation team 53 question 2: definition of teamwork brill (1976) states that teamwork has a variety of m e a n i n g s , but valid generalizations can be made about it. in his definition of teamwork, brill (1976, p.22) highlights four essential points. 1) a team is a group of individuals, each of whom is responsible for making decisions. 2) each particular member possesses particular expertise. 3) team members hold a common purpose. 4) team members communicate, collaborate and consolidate knowledge together from which plans are made, actions are determined and future decisions influenced. in analyzing the respondents' definitions of teamwork, many points consistent with those made by brill (1976) were evident. represented in table 2 below, are the pertable 1. definition of rehabilitation centages of respondents who included these points in their definitions. question 3a +3b : advantages of teamwork kane (1975, p.12) and brill (1976, p.26) delineate the following advantages of teamwork for professionals: 1) simplified access to and communication with other professionals. 2) promotion of learning. 3) awareness that the work of others can facilitate one's own work. 4) co-ordination of skilled services for the patient. 5) avoidance of duplication. 6) teamwork provides a more comprehensive but integrated range of services. 7) teamwork provides a forum for examination and evaluation of ideas in the light of the differing frames of reference of the various members. pt ot st 1. rehabilitation involves the treatment and the training of the patient so that he/she may obtain his/her maximal potential for normal living, given the constraints imposed by illness or injury. 61,5 % 70,0 % 23,8 % 2. rehabilitation embraces the physical, social, vocational and mental aspects of the patient's life. 73,1 % 30,0 % 19,0 % 3. the emphasis is on improving the patient's capability to function independently. 11,5 % 25,0 % 0,0 % 4. a habit of identifying potential compensating strengths in a person's functioning, as it is not what the patient has lost but what he has left that is important. 7,7 % 25,0 % 19,0 % 5. rehabilitation is undertaken by doctors in co-operation with physiotherapists, occupational therapists, social workers, speech-language therapists, psychologists and nurses in an attempt to treat a patient holistically. 7,7 % 10,0 % 4,8 % 6. returning the patient to a premorbid state of living as far as possible. 23,1 % 20,0 % 33,3 % percentages do not add up to 100 as respondents could include more than one feature in their definitions. table 2. definition of teamwork i pt ot st 1. teamwork involves a group of individuals. 42,3 % 65,0 % 71,4 % 2. teamwork involves co-operation, communication and collaboration of ideas and information. 42,3 % 20,0 % 28,6 % 3. each member has particular expertise in his/her field. 3,8 % 40,0 % 14,3 % 4. there are common goals, aims or plans of action. 30,8 % 10,0 % 28,6 % 5. teamwork is an approach that achieves optimal benefit for the patient. 3,8 % 20,0 % 28,6 % 6. there is complementation of different treatments, and reinforcement of behaviours taught in other therapies. 11,5 % 5,0 % 4,8 % 7. teamwork is a holistic approach to patient care. 0,0 % 5,0 % 0,0 % 8. teamwork includes the patient, his/her family, and other lay people. 7,7 % 35,0 % 23,8 % percentages do not add up to 100 as respondents could include more than one feature in their definitions. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 the responses to question 3a showed that the majority of respondents in each student group, were in agreement regarding the possible advantages of teamwork. the majority of respondents agreed that teamwork is an experience in participatory learning (3a.1), that teamwork provides for an exchange of ideas (3a.3) and that teamwork focuses on goals for mutual effort (3a.4). there was one hundred percent agreement across student groups that the holistic approach to teamwork (3a.2) is advantageous. finally, responses to statement 3a.5 (teamwork provides an overlap of services) showed greater variability. less than ten percent of respondents in each group were in disagreement with this statement. the majority of respondents (over eighty percent in all groups) however, were in agreement that the overlap of services in teamwork is beneficial. item 3b, an open-ended question, requested the respondents to add any further possible advantages of teamwork. forty-eight percent of the speech-language therapy students, 54 % of the physiotherapy students and 55 % of the occupational therapy students, all contributed further advantages, namely, there is a support system for professionals. goals are prioritised in terms of the whole patient. teamwork breaks down professional barriers. question 4a + 4b disadvantages of teamwork "it is naive to bring together a highly diverse group of people and expect that, by calling them a team they will in fact behave as a team" (wise, beckard, rubin & kyte, 1974, p.56). in the light of the above quote, it is important to acknowledge the possible problems that may arise when professionals work within a team. brill (1976), schlesinger (1985), and germain (1984) point out the major downfalls of teamwork, namely, 1) the team situation fosters undesirable competition. 2) problems of role definition and frequent overlap. 3) there are major problems of communication in teams. 4) team meetings are often time-consuming. 5) health professionals often have insufficient knowlisa felsher & eleanor ross ledge about the education and expertise of other disciplines. 6) health professionals tend to assign most tasks to their own profession rather than to others. 7) disagreement with the goals of treatment. the responses to question 4a, that consists of seven statements concerning the possible disadvantages of teamwork, showed greater variability relative to the responses to question 3a. approximately 70 % of respondents in each group were in agreement that a lack of knowledge of what other team members do (question 4a.5) is a disadvantage of teamwork. furthermore, the majority of respondents in each student group, were also in agreement that problems of role definition (question 4a.4) may be a possible downfall of teamwork. these findings highlight the need to educate students of all three professional disciplines, regarding the roles and functions of other rehabilitation specialities. the second part of question 4, required respondents to add any other possible disadvantages of teamwork. ten percent of both speech-language therapy and occupational therapy students, and four percent of physiotherapy students, contributed further disadvantages, namely, personality clashes between team members. professionals, for example doctors, may doubt the efficacy of the other professionals' therapies. there may be too many opinions. professionals may give contradictory suggestions for therapeutic intervention. question 5a and 5b according to galley & forster (1987), wade et al., (1985), and dean & geiringer (1990), the doctor or physician tends to be the leader of the team. bonner (1969) states that all members of the team look to the doctor for guidance, direction and enlightenment, while dean & geiringer (1990) propose that the physician is skilled in integrating a complex patient-care system. in contrast with the above view, wade et al., (1985) state that doctors take a less-than-active role, leaving the decio t s p t s s t s 8 5 % 88% 1 5 % 8 1 % 1 2 % ^ s h a r e d l e a d e r s h i p of the t e a m b y m o r e t h a n o n e m e m b e r . s o l e l e a d e r s h i p o n e m e m b e r of the t e a m a s s u m e s t h e l e a d e r s h i p role. figure 2. leadership of the rehabilitation team. / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the knowledge and attitudes of occupational therapy, physiotherapy & speech-language therapy students, regarding the speech-language therapist's role in the hospital stroke rehabilitation team 55 sions to the other team members. in a research study c o n d u c t e d by motona & nowitz ( 1 9 9 1 ) , qualified physiotherapists were asked to give their opinion regarding who should be primarily in charge of a rehabilitation team. the majority of respondents indicated that the physiotherapist should be in charge of the team, while approximately one third of the respondents believed that leadership should be shared. this finding suggests that there is a movement away from the doctor-led team. figure 2. depicts in the form of pie-charts, the percentages of respondents, who indicated that leadership should be shared by more than one professional of the team, and those who expressed that only one professional of the team should be in charge. the respondents who indicated that one professional should lead the team, mentioned the doctor, the social worker and the occupational therapist as being sole leaders. it is interesting to note that not one of the physiotherapy respondents indicated that the doctor should lead the rehabilitation team. this may possibly be due to the fact that physiotherapists do not require patient referrals from doctors (beenhakker, 1991), while under the current legislation, occupational therapists and speech-language therapists have to have their patients referred to them by a registered medical practitioner (south african medical and dental council, act 56). of the respondents who indicated that leadership should be shared among professionals, four main combinations of leaders emerged, namely 1) all members should share leadership. 2) the speech-language therapist, the occupational therapist, and the physiotherapist. 3) the speech-language therapist, the occupational therapist, the physiotherapist and the social worker. 4) the speech-language therapist, the occupational therapist, the physiotherapist and the psychologist. question 6a and 6b beenhakker (1991, p.8) suggests that "for professionals to be able to work together as a team, they need to study together as students." she believes that this encourages a greater understanding of health professionals' mutual problems, and will establish co-operative behaviour patterns. beenhakker (1991) further adds that to date, this is not being achieved at the university of the witwatersrand, medical school, and probably at very few other training institutions. beenhakker (1991, p.8) emphasises the need for students to campaign for a "multidisciplinary approach to teaching from their first year of study". only then may students learn from an early stage the "mechanics of multidisciplinary activities and the particular expertise of the individual professions." in a research study conducted by waller & murphy (1985, p. 19-22) qualified speech-language therapists who worked in hospital settings, were questioned as to whether they felt a need for more knowledge of rehabilitation specialities such as occupational therapy, physiotherapy and social work. approximately 50 % of the respondents indicated that iri "some cases", they could use more information on all rehabilitation specialists. in addition, when asked about course lectures received in their undergraduate training, regarding other rehabilitation specialists, 80,6 % of the respondents stated that no courses on occupational therapy were covered, while 77,6 % of the respondents indicated that courses on physiotherapy had not been given. question 6a requested respondents to indicate their attitudes to the statement read as follows: "for professionals to be able to work together as a team, they need to work together as students." the pie-charts in figure 3 indicate the percentage of respondents who were in agreement, disagreement and who were undecided about the above statement. o t s p t s s t s 95% 77% 1 5 % 100% figure 3. working together as students die suid-afrikaanse rif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 lisa felsher & eleanor ross speech-language therapy students unanimously agreed with the above statement. this may be attributed to the fact that speech-language therapy students at the university of the witwatersrand are not part of the medical campus and have no contact with occupational therapy and physiotherapy students. occupational therapy and physiotherapy students are both situated on the medical school campus, and have formal lectures together for the first two years of their four year course. occupational therapy and physiotherapy hospital departments are located in close physical proximity and thus more interaction occurs between these two professional disciplines. some of the comments made in agreement with the statement were as follows: it is important to build rapport and understanding of each other's professions at a student level. there is little communication between students of different disciplines. a basic foundation of mutual co-operation must be instilled in students. we must learn to respect and appreciate other disciplines. this is not being done at present, largely due to the lack of communication between members of staff of the various disciplines. i have never in my three and a half years worked with another student, nor received formal information on the roles of other professionals. some comments made by those respondents who were undecided about the statement were as follows :although professions overlap, there is no need for specific interaction. when qualified, it should be just as easy to move into a team approach. the following comments were made by those respondents who were in disagreement to question 6a. students are still trying to prove themselves to their peers. as professionals, people feel on a more equal footing. professionals can quickly learn to work together once qualified. section c bluestone (1976, p. 14) aptly sums up both the advantages and disadvantages of role duplication. she states that "because of the overlapping interests of the various professions, a definition of roles is needed to avoid both gaps and duplication". on the other hand, "cross-fertilisation of skills permits valuable reinforcement" of each professional's treatment by the other health professionals on the team, for example the social'worker and the speech-language therapist both may be counselling the patients about their reduced but active role in the family. wade et al., (1985, p.322) made the following suggestions regarding which team professionals would be responsible for different aspects of stroke rehabilitation, most of which were included in the research questionnaire. they saw the physiotherapist and the occupational therapist as being involved in the motor recovery of limbs. the occupational therapist, the physiotherapist and the nurse were the three team members seen to be responsible for rehabilitation of activities of daily living such as feeding, grooming and bathing. wade et al., (1985) further stated that the speech-language therapist, the psychologist and the social worker were responsible for emotional adjustment of the patient, while they suggested that the social worker, the psychologist, friends, religious advisors and the speechlanguage therapist were responsible for family and social support. the rehabilitation of communication problems was seen to be in the professional realm of both the speech-language therapist and the psychologist. finally, the social worker and the occupational therapist were the two members who were perceived to be involved in the patient's long-term occupation. ylvisaker and urbanczyk (1990) comment on cognitive rehabilitation, and view it as being an inter-disciplinary attempt to overcome cognitive obstacles which may result from a stroke, such as impairment of memory. while the role of the neuropsychologist in cognitive rehabilitation has been primarily diagnostic (turnbull, 1992), it is felt that the speech-language therapist and the occupational therapist fulfil a more therapeutic role in the rehabilitative efforts of impaired cognitive abilities. each of the seven aspects of stroke rehabilitation are discussed separately as follows. of those respondents who indicated that more than one professional is involved in motor recovery of limbs (question one), 60% of the occupational therapy students, 26,9% of the physiotherapy students and 14,3% of the speech-language therapy students, indicated that the occupational therapist and the physiotherapist would be jointly involved in this aspect of rehabilitation. in question two, 45% of the occupational therapy students, 27% of the physiotherapy students, and 14,3% of the speech-language therapy students, indicated that the occupational therapist is exclusively responsible for the rehabilitation of activities of daily living. twenty percent of the occupational therapy students and 11,5% of the physiotherapy students indicated that the speechlanguage therapist would have a role to play in this aspect of stroke rehabilitation, while almost half (47,6%) of the speech-language therapy students felt that they had a role to play in the rehabilitation of activities of daily living. this important finding may -possibly be attributed to the fact that the speech-language therapist is frequently involved in the diagnosis and treatment of dysphagia, which can be viewed as an essential function of daily living. in question three, a substantially large percentage of students across all three groups indicated that more than one professional would be responsible for the emotional adjustment of the patient. this finding may possibly be attributed to the fact that all members of the team receive some form of training in counselling skills, in their student education. ^ it is interesting to note that for the rehabilitation of communication problems (in question 4), substantially higher percentages of occupational therapy and physiotherapy respondents indicated that only one professional, i.e., the speech-language therapist, should be involved in this aspect. in comparison to these findings, 71,4% of the speech-language therapy students indithe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the knowledge and attitudes of occupational therapy, physiotherapy & speech-language therapy students, regarding the speech-language therapist's role in the hospital stroke rehabilitation team 57 cated that rehabilitation of communication problems involved more than one team member. this finding suggests that speech-language therapy students do not see themselves as exclusively responsible for the rehabilitation of communication disorders, but rather see other team members as also being valuable contributors to communication rehabilitation. question 5 dealt with the aspect of family and social support. while 25% of the occupational therapy students, 19% of the physiotherapy students and 9,5% of the speech-language therapy students, indicated that the social worker is exclusively responsible for this aspect, there was a substantially high percentage of respondents in each group who felt that family and social support is given by more than one professional. it is interesting to note that more than half (54,4%) of the speech-language therapy respondents felt that they had a role to play in family and social support, while only 30% of the occupational therapy students and 34,6% of the physiotherapy students mentioned the speech therapist's role in this aspect. of those respondents who indicated that more than one professional is responsible for cognitive rehabilitation (question 6), 30% of the occupational therapy students and 23% of the physiotherapy students indicated that the occupational therapist and the speech-language therapist are jointly responsible. nineteen percent of the speech-language therapy students mentioned that in addition to the occupational and speech-language therapists, the family and the psychologist are also responsible for rehabilitation of impaired cognitive abilities. it is interesting that an extremely high percentage (95,2) of speech-language therapy students felt that they had a role to play in cognitive rehabilitation, compared with 61,5% of the physiotherapy students and only 35% of the occupational therapy students who indicated the involvement of the speech-language therapist. the reason for only 35% of occupational therapy students mentioning the speech-language therapist's role in cognitive rehabilitation, may possibly be attributed to the fact that occupational therapy students view cognitive rehabilitation as falling within their own professional realm. j the final aspect of stroke rehabilitation is that of long-term occupation (question 7). haydock (1992) states that the patient's ability to return to his or her previous employment mayj have to be assessed by the occupational therapist. thirty-five percent of the occupational therapy students, and 31% of the physiotherapy students mentioned that both the occupational therapist and the social worker need to be involved in longterm occupation of the patient, while 14,3% of the speech-language therapy students indicated that the family should be involved together with the occupational therapist and the social worker. it is important to note that 35% of the occupational therapy students saw their profession as being exclusively involved in long-term occupation. it is also interesting to note that 33,3% of the speech-language therapy students mentioned their involvement, together with other team members, in the aspect of long-term occupation . this finding may possibly be attributed to the fact that communication disabilities often impinge upon vocational aspects of the patient's life. section d the role of the speechlanguage therapist as mentioned previously, four of the most common disorders resulting from a stroke, in which the speechlanguage therapist plays an active role are aphasia, dysarthria, apraxia and dysphagia. logemann (1990, p.157) states that the role of the speech-language therapist in dysphagia encompasses evaluation and treatment of dysphagic patients. in question 3, there was one hundred percent agreement from both occupational and speech-language therapy students, and 96,2% agreement from the physiotherapy students, regarding the active role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of dysphagia. penn (1992), simmons (1981), clifford-rose & capildeo (1981) and softley (1987), all stress the active role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of aphasia. similarly, respondents in each student group were all in total agreement regarding the role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of aphasia. as with the treatment of aphasia, penn (1992), simmons (1981), clifford-rose & capildeo (1981) and softley (1987) all emphasise the role that the speechlanguage therapist plays in the diagnosis and treatment of verbal apraxia or apraxia of speech. one hundred percent of both occupational therapy and speech-language therapy students agreed that the speech-language therapist is actively involved in the treatment of verbal apraxia, while only 84% of the physiotherapy students were in agreement. one hundred percent agreement of occupational therapy students in comparison with 84,6% agreement of the physiotherapy students, may possibly be attributed to the fact that occupational therapy students diagnose and treat other types of apraxia such as dressing apraxia, limb apraxia, and constructional apraxia (thompson & morgan 1990). the role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of dysarthria, has also been highlighted by various authors including penn (1992) and softley (1987). in question 12, there is unanimous agreement on the part of both occupational and speech-language therapy students, while 96,2% of the physiotherapy students agreed that the speech-language therapist plays a role in the treatment of dysarthria. thus, it appears that both occupational therapy and physiotherapy students are well informed, with regard to the four main disorders post-stroke, with which a speech-language therapist would be actively involved. in addition, these four most common disorders are intensively covered in the latter two years of the fourth year speech-language therapy course. it is interesting to note that, while both occupational and physiotherapy students were in one hundred percent agreement regarding the role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of auditory agnosia (question 14), only 80,9% of the speech-language therapy students agreed with question 14's statement. auditory agnosia, together with visual object agnosia, visual spatial agnosia and tactile agnosia, is a disorder of perception that may be present following a stroke (thompson & morgan, 1990). the lower percentage of agreement within the speech-language therapy group, may possibly be attributed to the fact that auditory die suid-afrikaanse rif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 lisa felsher & eleanor ross agnosia and the clinical treatment thereof, is less intensively covered in their academic curriculum. in addition there appears to be a paucity of literature, covering the role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of auditory agnosia. to date, there appears to be lack of clarity regarding the role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of agraphia as well as alexia. while both disorders imply a loss or impairment of some mode of communication, the speech-language therapist's role in such disorders is unclear. penn (1992) differentiates between difficulties of reading and writing that arise from different problems. reading and writing difficulties that arise from sensori-motor deficits such as hemianopsia and hemiplegia, must not be confused with reading and writing problems that are a result of difficulty in comprehending or producing the written word respectively. this suggests that both the speech-language therapist and the occupational therapist should have a role to play in the treatment of alexia and agraphia. furthermore, reading and writing difficulties that arise from sensorimotor deficits should fall within the professional realm of the occupational therapist, whereas reading and writing difficulties that are a function of a greater language impairment should fall within the ambit of speech-language therapy. in questions four and five, occupational therapy students in comparison with physiotherapy and speech-language therapy students were in the least agreement, and were the most undecided about the speech-language therapist's role in the treatment of agraphia and alexia respectively. it is interesting to note that 76,2% of the speech-language therapy students agreed that they had a role to play in the treatment of agraphia, while 85,7% of the speech-language therapy students were of the opinion that they could contribute to the treatment of alexia. question 11 dealt with the role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of amnesia. the view that cognitive rehabilitation, which includes the treatment of amnesia, should be a broad-based, functional and inter-disciplinary effort (ylvisaker & urbanczyk 1990) suggests that more than one professional discipline has a role to play in cognitive rehabilitation. it is interesting to note that, while 100% of the speech-language therapy students felt that their profession had a part to play in the treatment of amnesia, only 30% of the occupational therapy students were in agreement with this. this finding is consistent with that of question 6 in section c, which suggests that occupational therapy students may view the treatment of amnesia (an aspect of cognitive rehabilitation), within their own professional field. similarly, speech-language therapy students may see the treatment of amnesia, as exclusively belonging to their discipline. conclusion in summary, results of this study suggested a fairly good understanding of the concepts of rehabilitation and teamwork. however, speech-language therapy students provided the narrowest definition of rehabilitation suggesting a possible under-emphasis of this concept in their clinical and theoretical training. students appeared to have a greater understanding of those disorders post-stroke, with which a speech-language therapist is commonly involved. however, students showed less understanding of those disorders following a stroke, for which the speech-language therapist's role is less clearly defined. in addition, a high percentage of role duplication/overlapping in the several aspects of stroke rehabilitation was found. these results hold important implications for the profession of speech-language pathology in the 1990's in terms of undergraduate and postgraduate inter-disciplinary education, clarification of the speech-language therapist's clinical role, public relations, and for further research. inter-disciplinary education inter-disciplinary education at both undergraduate and postgraduate levels should be a joint and reciprocal way in which health care professionals can learn from and about each other. this could be achieved in a number of possible ways, namely, i) inter-departmental case presentations on a regular basis whereby cases would be presented from different frames of reference. ii) collaborative research involving students from vari\ \ ous disciplines. iii) joint rural/community blocks at present, only final year occupational therapy and physiotherapy students attend a three-week rural block. iv) seminars or a series of lectures covering other paramedical professionals' roles in stroke and other rehabilitation. v) improving inter-departmental links between both staff members and students alike. vi) setting up student rehabilitation clinics, allowing students to work in a holistic environment. speech-language therapist's clinical role the results of this study suggested that occupational therapy, physiotherapy and speech-language therapy students acknowledged the role of the speech-language therapist in the treatment of such disorders as aphasia, dysarthria, verbal apraxia and dysphagia] with which the speech-language therapist is commonly associated. however, students showed less understanding of such disorders as amnesia, alexia and agraphia, for which the speech-language therapist's role is not as well defined. this finding implies the need to clarify the speech-language therapist's clinical role, as well as other paramedical therapist's roles in such disorders. / public relations there appears to be the need for speech-language and hearing therapists to market their profession. van hattum (1980) maintains that it is ironic that a profession devoted to the treatment of communication disor' der has had so much difficulty communicating this information. thus, the profession of speech-language and hearing therapy needs to perform an,educative role, which could be achieved in a number of ways: 1) through academic contributions to medical and paramedical journals of south africa.. 2) through explanatory pamphlets or audio-visual media outlining the role of the speech-language and hearing therapist in the treatment of different comthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the knowledge a n d attitudes of occupational therapy, physiotherapy & speech-language t h e r a p y students, regarding the speech-language therapist's role in the hospital stroke rehabilitation t e a m 59 munication disorders. 3) through workshops, orientation programmes or professional forums for medical and paramedical students and professionals throughout their programs. suggestions for further research 1. replication of the present study, using a similar or the same questionnaire, on qualified occupational, speech and physiotherapists, to establish the extent to which their attitudes and knowledge are related to increased experience. 2. evaluation of an existing inter-disciplinary stroke rehabilitation team, to assess the effectiveness of such an approach for future teamwork practise. 3. investigation of the knowledge and attitudes of final year social work and/or nursing students, as well as medical doctors specialising in neurology, regarding the role of the speech-language therapist in the hospital stroke rehabilitation team. 4. investigation of the attitudes and knowledge of medical doctors specialising in ear, nose and throat surgery, towards the speech-language and hearing therapist's role in the cleft-palate or laryngectomy team. 5. investigation of t h e attitudes and k n o w l e d g e o f occupational t h e r a p y and p h y s i o t h e r a p y students, reg a r d i n g the s p e e c h l a n g u a g e therapist's role in the i n t e r d i s c i p l i n a r y m a n a g e m e n t o f c e r e b r a l p a l s i e d children. brill ( 1 9 7 6 , p. 143) m a i n t a i n s that " t h e great need o f the student in intraand intergroup relations is not only for k n o w l e d g e o f the g r o u p s i n v o l v e d but also for the ability to r e a c h a c r o s s t h e s e b o u n d a r i e s w h i l e at the s a m e time r e s p e c t i n g t h e m " . it is hoped t h a t this study will p r o m o t e a g e n e r a l a w a r e n e s s of the need to gain k n o w l e d g e a n d u n d e r s t a n d i n g of the roles that v a r i o u s p a r a m e d i c a l professionals p l a y in stroke rehabilitation and u l t i m a t e l y lead to m o r e effective and holistic p a tient m a n a g e m e n t . j i r e f e r e n c e s beenhakker, j.c. (1991). role of the physiotherapist in the health team. the leech 60,2,8 -9. bluestone, s.s. (1976). rehabilitation medicine. in haller, r.m. & sheldon, n. (eds.) speech pathology and audiology in medical settings. new york: stratton intercontinental medical book corporation! bonner, d. (ed.) (1969). the\team approach to hemiplegia. illinois: charles thomas publisher. boone, p.r. (1987). human communication and its disorders. new jersey: prentice hall incorporated. brill, n.i. (1976). teamwork working together in the human services. philadelphia: j.b. lippincott company. chamberlain, m.a. (1989). what is rehabilitation ?br. j. hosp. med., 41,311. clifford-rose, c. & capildeo, r. (1981) stroke: the facts. oxford university: new york press. dean, b.z. & geiringer, s.r. (1990). psychiatric therapeutics. 6. the rehabilitation team/ behavioural management. arch. phys. med. rehabil., 71, 4, s-275 s-276. fritz, v. (1992). the medical perspective. in fritz, v. & penn.c. (eds). stroke: caring and coping. j o h a n n e s b u r g : witwatersrand medical press. fritz, v. & penn,c. (eds) (1992). stroke: caring and coping. johannesburg: witwatersrand medical press. galley, p.m. & forster, a.l. (1987). human movement: an introductory text for physiotherapy students. second edition, new york: churchill livingstone. germain, c.b. (1984). social work practise in health care: an ecological perspective. new york: the free press. gloag, d. (1985). severe disability: tasks of rehabilitation. br. med. j., 290, 1333-1336. gresham, g.e. (1986). the rehabilitation of the stroke survivor. in barnett, h.j.m. (ed). stroke: pathophysiology, diagnosis and management. new york: churchill livingstone. haller, r.m. & sheldon, n. (1976). speech pathology and audiology in medical settings. new york: stratton intercontinental medical book corporation. haydock, s. (1992). occupational therapy. in fritz, v. & penn, c. (eds) stroke caring and coping. j o h a n n e s b u r g : witwatersrand university press. kane,r.a. (1975) interprofessional teamwork. syracuse: new york syracuse university press. logemann, j. (1990). dysphagia. sem. speech lang. 11,3,157163. love, r.j. & webb, w.g. (1986). neurology for the speechlanguage pathologist. boston: butterworth publishers. milner, m.(1989). knowledge and attitudes of fifth year medical students concerning the profession of speech therapy and audiology. unpublished honours research project. department of speech pathology and audiology: university of the witwatersrand. motona, b.s. & nowitz, l.m. (1991). physiotherapists' perceptions of rehabilitation. unpublished honours r e s e a r c h project. d e p a r t m e n t of p h y s i o t h e r a p y : university of the witwatersrand. n i c h o l s , p.j.r. (1976). rehabilitation medicine: the management of physical disabilities. london: butterworth publishers. penn, c.(1992). management of communication problems. in fritz, v. & penn, c. (eds). stroke: caring and coping. johannesburg: witwatersrand university press. purtilo, r. (1988). ethical issues in teamwork: the context of rehabilitation. arch. phys. med. rehabil. 69, 5, 318-322. schlesinger,e.g.(1985).hea/ intersystem interactions implications of results * key: equipment used for tile hearing assessment included the weiss neometer providing calibrated high and low frequency narrowband noise for behaviour observation audiometry and the gsi 28aauto tymp (grason-stadler, inc) for the evaluation of middle-ear functioning. figure i: neonatal communication assessment model for biologically at-risk infants results and discussion figure ii presents an overall view of the average functioning of the 50 subjects for the eight areas of assessment. according to figure ii the subjects presented with normal levels of functioning (values of 7 to 10 according to the rating scale as suggested by allen & alexander, 1992) for hearing abilities, middle-ear functioning, motor and oral-motor skills, whereas precursors for cognition and language use, form and content displayed a delay in development. precursors for language use can be singled out as the developmental area displaying the greatest delay. the subjects' normal developmental levels are comparable to results of research done by aylward, gustafson, verhulst & colliver (1987) and gorga, stern, nass & nagler (1988) for subjects | ranging from minimal to extreme prematurity. these studies found that most subjects displayed no neurological or motor disorders in the neonatal period. studies on extremely premature neonates (majnemer et al., 1992; piper, kunos, willis & mazer, 1985; tbuwen, 1990) however, found large differences in general development in comparison with fullterm neonates. it therefore appears that the more premature the neonates are, the greater the delay in their general development. the delays in cognitive and communication development displayed by the subjects indicate limited interaction with their environment and caregivers. these findings are recognized by various researchers as typical neonatal behaviour of biologically at-risk infants (als, 1986; brazelton, 1984; stjernqvist & svenningsen, 1990). it is suggested that these disordered interaction patterns are related to sensory overstimulation, absence of contingent interaction with caregivers and a lack of rhythmicity and routines in the nicu (als, 1986). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 alta kritzinger, brenda louw and ιιβηέ hugo figure iii presents the results of the spearman correlation coefficients conducted to determine significant correlations (<0,5) between the subjects' developmental levels across the different developmental areas. all significant correlations that occurred were positive, indicating the subjects' improvement or delay in one developmental area displayed an improvement or delay in the correlated developmental area as well. according to figure iii significant positive correlations occurred between the assessment areas of the communication protocol representing different aspects of behaviour (motor and oral-motor skills and precursors of cognition, language use, form and content). those assessment areas measuring physiological d e v e l o p m e n t a l level 10 9 1 8 -76 i i i i 5 4 3 2 1 1 1 1 i i 1 1 1 1 m 1 it 1 fa • it 1 η mel μ om c lc li lu 9.64 7.46 7.56 6.92 5.01 4.98 5.21 3.52 . a s s e s s m e n t a r e a s figure ii: average developmental levels of subjects, n=50 key: h= hearing abilities; mef= middle ear functioning; m= motor skills; om= oral-motor skills; c= precursors of cognition; lc= precursors of language content; lf= precursors of language form; lu= precursors of language use θ λ (me) v j i l l figure iii: significant correlations between the results of the different developmental areas, n=50 key: h= hearing abilities; me= middle ear functioning; m= motor skills; om= oral-motor skills; c= precursors of cognition; lc= precursors of language content; lf= precursors of language form; lu= precursors of language use functioning (the subjects' hearing abilities and middle-ear functioning) did not indicate any intersystem interactions as they require different parameters of assessment compared to behaviour. a detailed analysis of the results based on figure ii and figure iii is supplied by discussing each developmental area separately. general developmental systems: intersystem analysis and interactions hearing abilities the subjects' response levels were indicative of normal hearing abilities in a non-soundproof room. no sensoryneural hearing loss was detected among the subjects and risk factors such as the noise levels in the nicu, the use of ototoxic drugs (see table 2) and all other risk conditions described in the high risk criteria for the identification of hearing loss in children (asha, 1989) did not seem to have affected their hearing abilities. it was found that doctors in the hospitals studied, carefully monitored the ototoxic levels in the blood when such drugs were used and often avoided using these drugs. it could therefore be that reported incidence of sensory-neural hearing loss between 2% and 10% among the nicu graduates (salamy & eldridge, 1991), served to focus the attention on preventative measures of which we see the results now already. middle-ear functioning three different aspects of the results on the middle-ear functioning of the subjects are discussed, namely abnormal middle-ear measurements, acoustic reflexes and tympanograms. the measurement of 7,48 (see figure ii) indicates normal middle-ear functioning for the group as a whole. 20% of the subjects, however, displayed abnormal middle-ear measurements for static compliance, middle-ear pressure and the shape of the tympanogram. these findings seem to indicate an increased incidence of abnormal middle-ear functioning among the subjects when compared with studies on middle-ear functioning of fullterm neonates, which found the incidence of middle-ear pathology between 0% (zarnoch & balkany, 1977) and 4,1% (mann, 1986) in that population. it has been suggested that abnormal middleear functioning is related to the prolonged use of nasogastric tube feeding resulting in decreased ventilation of the middle-ear as no swallowing is required for this kind of feeding. there is, however, no clarity about the nature of the abnormal middle-ear measurements displayed by the subjects as these neonates have just completed courses of antibiotics that would have cleared up otitis media with effusion as well. clinical experience indicates the difficulty of diagnosing abnormal middle-ear functioning as the premature external meatus is very narrow and limits the visual inspection of the tympanic membrane. it is therefore suggested that increased fluid in the middle-ear as measured in 20% of the subjects may be sterile, but can still cause a conductive hearing loss. although this hearing loss will only influence communication development when it recurs frequently, early detection of abnormal middle-ear functioning in biologically at-risk neonates may lead to the early identification of those who are otitis media-prone. further research is necessary to establish if this the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication skills of biologically at-risk neonates 13 is indeed the way to identify otitis media-prone infants at biological risk. early identification of otitis media in biologically at-risk neonates can ensure early treatment resulting in decreasing the negative effects on their communication development as various studies have already found a high incidence of otitis media among biologically at-risk infants (pearce et al., 1988). the subjects' acoustical reflexes displayed normal values but could only be elicited after an average of four attempts. these findings are in agreement with other studies finding neonatal acoustical reflexes difficult to elicit (mann, 1986; walters & shimizu, 1990). finally, 94% of the subjects displayed type d and type ε tympanograms indicating highly mobile middle-ear systems. research done by keith (1975) and mann (1986) on fullterm neonates and zarnoch and balkany (1977) on biologically at-risk neonates indicates a high incidence of mobile middle-ear systems that can be related to the incomplete ossification of the middle-ear system in fullterm neonates as well as in premature neonates. the incidence of mobile middle-ear systems in these three studies, however, are not as high as in the present study. the findings of the present study on the mobility of the subjects' middle-ear systems are therefore only partly in agreement with previous studies. motor akilla the subjects' average motor skills were within normal limits and are comparable to the studies of aylward et al. (1987) and gorga et al. (1988). uncontrolled body movements and lack of an overall extension pattern can be singled out as the subjects' typical motor behaviour. according to morris and klein (1987) the lack of a physiological flexion pattern of the body is characteristic of premature neonates as they are born too soon to be forced into flexion due to decreased intra-uterine space. oral-motor skills the subjects' oral-motor skills displayed values just below the average (6,92). the| subjects were assessed at a stage when they had already overcome most of their feeding problems and shortly before discharge from the nicu. table 1 no 8 indicates that the| subjects did experience feeding problems prior to assessment, some to the extent that it took them 75 days to bottle-feed satisfactorily. a qualitative analysis of the results revealed depressed rooting reflexes, prolonged dependence on nasogastric tube-feeding (44% of subjects) and prolonged duration of feeding (only 28% of the subjects couldcomplete their feeding within 20 minutes) as the remainder of their feeding difficulties. the feeding difficulties experienced by the subjects are similar to those described by bu'lock et al. (1990), jaffe (1989) and morris and klein (1987). it is suggested the feeding difficulties of biologically at-risk neonates are related to neurological disorganization and the predominant extension pattern of the body. these results, supported by the spearman correlation coefficients (see figure iii) indicated that relationships exist between the subjects' motor and oral-motor skills, therefore demonstrating interactions within the motor system. precursors of cognitive skills the results indicate that the subjects evidenced a delay in the development of precursors of cognitive development. on average the subjects displayed difficulties with actively attending to environmental stimuli. according to brazelton (1984) the neonates' characteristic ability to control stimuli by changing their levels of consciousness (from sleep to alertness) may be the most predictive aspect of their behaviour. the subjects' passive state can therefore be ascribed to their limited.ability for organization and intergration of stimuli within and from the environment. these findings are also supported by majnemer et al. (1992) and stjernqvist and svenningsen (1990). communication system: intrasystem analysis and interactions precursors of language use the results of the development of the precursors of language use revealed the subjects' lowest level of development of all areas assessed. characteristic neonatal communication interaction behaviour such as eye contact, imitation of facial expressions and synchronized interactions occurred rarely. these results display clear intersystem interactions between the cognitive and communicative systems (see figure iii as well) of the subjects and can be explained in the same way as the subjects' passive responses to non-social stimuli (development of precursors of cognition). the development of the precursors of language use indicates that the subjects' social interaction with the environment was poor. this can be related to the inconsistent and limited social interaction provided by the nicu environment. comparison of the subjects' severe delay in developing precursors for language use to the other results, appear to support the opinion of rosenblith (1992) namely, that infants at biological risk exhibit a specific vulnerability for communication disorders. when taken into consideration that precursors of language use are the first forms of communication to develop (lahey, 1988), the results of this study may point to some of the underpinnings of the communication disorders of biologically at-risk infants. precursors of language form as indicated in figure ii the subjects displayed a delay in the development of precursors of language form. it seemed that their typical vocal behaviour was still undifferentiated and reflexive in nature, again indicating limited interaction with the environment. these results are supported by morris and klein (1987) by their description of the limited vocal behaviour of biologically at-risk neonates as they do not cry to signal for hunger. the subjects' results regarding the development of precursors for language form displayed significant positive correlations with their oral-motor skills (see figure iii). these findings indicate that interactions between the motor and communication systems occurred, which is also recognized in the literature (morris & klein, 1987). precursors of language content although the subjects displayed normal hearing abilities, their development of listening skills as precursors of language content demonstrated delay. listening behaviour, distinguished by sustained attention, arrest of activity and alertness (brazelton, 1973) was seldomly observed. the lack of these responses also demonstrates the subjects' limited interaction with the social and non-social environment, thus possibly explaining the existence of the correlations displayed between the development of predie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 alta kritzinger, brenda louw and hugo cursors of language use and cognition (see figure iii). the results of the subjects' general and communication development indicated interactions across developmental systems as well as intrasystem interactions. this serves as a means to provide a wide angle perspective on the subjects' communication development. the results also indicate the synergy of the different developmental systems and demonstrates the difficulties that the subjects experienced to gain control over their developmental systems by means of regulation and organization of their behaviour. in summary, the subjects gave the impression of a neonate with normal hearing abilities, but a high risk for otitis media. s(he) mainly lies quietly in an extension pattern and has not fully mastered independent feeding skills. this neonate is mostly in a semi-alert state, pays attention to auditory and visual stimuli momentarily, cries or vocalizes seldom and attends only fleetingly to human contact and voices. this neonate is tiny, vulnerable, passive and has insufficient skills for communication interaction when compared to his / her fullterm counterpart who has been "prewired" (owens, 1984, p21) for communication. identification of risk factors relating to the subjects' communication development according to rossetti (1993) the early identification of communication disorders of biologically at-risk neonates is one of the most important factors contributing to the success of early communication intervention. the use of a high risk register for communication disorders implies that biologically at-risk neonates who are at special risk for communication disorders, can be identified before being discharged from the nicu and early communication intervention can continue without any interruption between neonatal ̂ nd postnatal intervention. a stepwise regression analysis was done in order to identify significant independent variables (on a 5% level of significance) correlating with the subjects' delayed communication development. thirteen significant independent variables were identified as significant risk factors that predicted the subjects' delayed communication development. this implies that the presence of a risk factor had a negative influence on the subjects' communication functioning. table 3 provides these risk factors in order of predictability as well as the different aspects of communication development that they were related to. according to table 3 ten risk factors were directly related to the subjects' biological conditions. birthweight and prematurity are among these risk factors, but they were not the strongest predictors of the subjects' commutable 3: significant risk factors of communication development in order of predictability, n=50 risk factor type of risk communication behaviour 1. success with bottle feeding biological risk precursors of language use precursors of cognition oral motor skills motor skills 2. receive breast milk environmental risk precursors of language content precursors of language form motor skills 3. frequency of parental visits in nicu environmental risk oral motor skills motor skills 4. gestation age (prematurity) biological risk precursors of language use ' precursors of language form 1 5. small-for-gestational-age biological risk precursors of language form [ middle-ear functioning j 6. gender (masculine) biological risk middle-ear functioning precursors of language form 7. birthweight biological risk oral motor skills 8. multiple births (twins) biological risk middle-ear functioning 9. perinatal infection biological risk precursors of language content 10. intracranial haemorrhage biological risk precursors of language f o r m / 11. apnoea attacks biological risk precursors of cognition 12. respiratory distress syndrome biological risk precursors of language content 13. non-infective conditions established risk middle-ear functioning the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication skills of biologically at-risk neonates 15 nication functioning as regarded in the literature (rossetti, 1993). the strongest predictor of the subjects' communication development was their success with bottle-feeding, as measured by the number of days taken to achieve successful bottle-feeding. the quicker the subjects could feed independently, the better their communication development was. the subjects' success with bottle-feeding predicted their functioning on a wide range of developmental areas, namely social and non-social interaction skills as well as motor and oral motor development, representing the communication, cognitive and motor systems. this indicates that feeding skills could be singled out as a basic prerequisite for the subjects' communication development. these findings are in agreement with alexander (1990) who considers impaired vegetative functioning as one of the earliest signs of developmental disorders. in considering all the different risk factors indicated in table 3, lack of success with bottle-feeding was the only parameter that provided information about the subjects' developmental status. some of the other biological risk factors (no 9 to 12) merely indicated the subjects' recovery from perinatal conditions, whereas birthweight, prematurity, gender and multiple births indicate well-known risks, but did not indicate how individual neonates react to these conditions. table 3 also indicates that environmental risk factors, as measured by parental involvement, negatively influenced the subjects' communication development. the parents' involvement was measured by providing breast milk for feeding and the frequency of their visits to the nicu. those subjects who received breast milk (mostly expressed breast milk received via tube feeding or bottlefeeding) benefitted from the interaction with mothers as these mothers had to bring the milk to the nicu daily and often fed the subjects themselves. the parents' regular involvement with the subjects in the nicu had a positive effect on aspects of their communication development as well as their motor and oral motor skills. the importance of parental involvement in the nicu is also stressed by various authors (als, 1986; jacobson & shubat, 1991; sparks, 1989). the last parameter indicated in table 3 were conditions that represent established risks (also see table 1 no 20) and indicates the close relationship existing between biological risks and established risks. established risk conditions can have their origin in prenatal stress conditions and are usually associated with retarded intrauterine growth. this implies that the risk status of an infant at biological risk can already begin during prenatal development. the established risk conditions as identified in this study, however, had a limited influence on the subjects' communication development, namely on their middle-ear functioning. the thirteen risk conditions as indicated in table 3 can therefore be considered as high risk factors for the subjects' neonatal communication development and are presented in table 4. the predictability of these risk factors for later communication development must still be established by further research in the form of longitudinal studies. in conclusion, the results of the study highlighted certable 4: high risk register for the prediction of the subjects' communication development, n=50 parameter risk factor 1. feeding the greater the number of days before successful bottle feeding, the greater the risk 2. receiving breastmilk in >nicu j ί mother not supplying breast milk implies less communication interaction with one caregiver and a risk for communication delay 3. parental visits in nicuj the lower the frequency of parental visits, the greater the risk 4. gestation age j the lower the gestation age, the greater the risk 5. small-for-gestational age | the presence of intra-uterine growth retardation implies a risk for communication delay / 6. gender boys displayed as greater risk for communication delays 7. birthweight the lower the birthweight, the greater the risk 8. multiple births twins displayed a greater risk for communication delay 9. perinatal infection the presence of an infection implies a risk for communication delay 10. intracranial haemorrhage the presence implies a risk for communication delay 11. apnoea attacks the presence implies a risk for communication delay 12. respiratory distress syndrome the presence implies a risk for communication delay 13. non-infectious conditions 1 the presence of established risk factors implies a risk for communication delay die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 tain aspects of neonatal communication abilities of biologically at-risk infants such as: the incidence of sensory neural hearing loss. the results of the study add to the unsolved question in the literature about the incidence of sensory-neural hearing loss among the nicu population; neonatal middle-ear functioning of infants at biological risk and the fact that the literature provides very limited guidelines for neonatal tympanometry. further research into the early identification of otitis prone infants at biological risk is necessary; the serious delay in communication interaction skills compared to other aspects of language development and general development. the effects of biological as well as environmental risk factors on neonatal communication abilities as evidenced in the proposed high risk register. it therefore appears that the results of the study, as obtained from the proposed assessment protocol, provided comprehensive information about the communication abilities of a group of biologically at-risk neonates, both from a speech-language pathology perspective as well as from an audiological viewpoint. conclusions neonatal communication intervention is the first link in the chain of continuous service delivery from birth to schoolgoing age to biologically at-risk infants and their families. this study focused attention on the earliest communication abilities of biologically at-risk infants. such an approach contributes to a perspective that retrospective studies, which have dominated this field of study in the past, cannot provide. the neonatal perspective underscores the detrimental influence of the nicu environment on the communication development of biologically at-risk neonates. the results of the study suggest that the noise levels in the nicu may pose a risk for hearing loss as well as interference with communication interaction. the results of the study also indicate that appropriate parental involvement in the nicu can make a difference to these neonates' early communication skills. a neonatal perspective further provides first-hand knowledge of the different biological and established risk conditions as well as neonatal communication behaviour and the interactions of these two aspects with one another. these aspects need to be explained to parents to ensure bonding and effective parent-infant communication interaction when the neonate displays readiness. it is therefore clear that a neonatal perspective on the communication abilities of biologically at-risk infants can contribute to the effectiveness of early intervention and adds a new dimension to the role of the early communication interventionist. the involvement of the speech-language therapist and audiologist in the multidisciplinary neonatal intervention team implies an extension of the field of communication pathology demanding specialized knowledge and clinical skills. this has implications for undergraduate training as well for continuing education of qualified professionals. another implication of the speech-language therapist's and audiologist's involvement in neonatal intervention is alta kritzinger, brenda louw and hugo the opportunity to provide services on the level of secondary prevention of communication disorders and in this way responding to the objectives of early communication intervention, i.e., preventing minor problems from becoming serious and new problems from developing (asha, 1991). the current study provides a tool for screening by means of the proposed high risk register (see table 4) as well as a neonatal communication assessment protocol for comprehensive evaluation purposes. the proposed high risk register for identification of those biologically at-risk neonates who are also at risk for communication disorders is a first attempt at such a strategy of early detection of communication pathology. the validity and reliability of the high risk register must, however, still be established as in the case of the well-known high risk criteria for the identification of hearing loss in children (asha, 1989). although a high risk register will always have limitations as there are still many immeasurable causes of communication pathology, it can be of much clinical value. the specific items of the proposed high risk register, especially the neonate's success with bottle feeding (see table 4), requires information that is readily available from the neonate's case history file or from the parents. the high risk register can therefore be a cost effective screening instrument to be used in primary health care in south africa. in a trans disciplinary context, community nurses may be trained to identify those neonates exhibiting risk factors for follow-up by early communication intervention specialists. this study provides a clinical assessment tool for the evaluation of neonatal communication abilities of biologically at-risk infants. the neonatal communication assessment protocol is as far as is known, unique in south africa. it is also a first attempt to objectify neonatal intervention by means of assessment. neonatal intervention in the south african context, however, still poses many challenges. this research project focused only on biologically at-risk neonates and did not study those who are also environmentally at-risk. further research needs to be conducted regarding the concerns of the most needy of the south african biologically at-risk population. additional research will enhance the development of early communication intervention in south africa, thereby giving priority to a strategy for the secondary prevention of communication disorders. early communication intervention is a key strategy that needs to be employed to cope with the large numbers of clients and the limited number of speech-language therapists and audiologists. correspondence should be addressed to: ι alta kritzinger, department of communication pathology, university of pretoria, pretoria 0002 references alexander, r. (1990). oral-motor and respiratory-phonatory assessment. in gibbs, e.d. & teti d.m. (eds.)interdisciplinary assessment of infants. baltimore: paul h. brooks publishing company. allen, m.c. & alexander, g.r. (1992). using gross motor milestones to identify very preterm infants at risk for cerebral palsy. developmental medicine and child neurology, 34:226232. als, h. (1986). a synactive model of neonatal behavioral organization: framework for the assessment of neurobehavioral development in the premature infant and for the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication skills of biologically at-risk neonates 17 support of infants and parents in the neonatal intensive care environment. in sweeny j.k. (ed.) the high-risk neonate: developmental therapy perspectives. new york: the haworth press. asha (1989). audiologic screening of infants who are at risk for hearing impairment. asha, march:89-92. asha(1991)· the prevention of communication disorders tutorial. asha supplement, 33(9): 15-39. aylward, g.p., gustafson, n., verhulst, s.j. & colliver, j.a. (1987). consistency in the diagnosis of cognitive, motor and neurologic function over the first three years. journal of pediatric psychology, 12(l):77-98. boettcher, f.a., henderson, d., gratton, m.a., danielson, r.w. & byrne, c.d. (1987). synergistic interactions of noise and other ototraumatic agents. ear and hearing, 8(4):192-211. brazelton, t.b. (1973). neonatal behavioral assessment scale. london: william heinemann medical books ltd. brazelton, t.b. (1984). neonatal behavioral assessment scale. london: blackwell scientific publications ltd. brazelton, t.b. (1987). behavioral competence of the newborn infant. in avery, g.b. (ed.) neonatology: pathophysiology and' the management of the newborn. philadelphia: j.b. lippincott. brazelton, t.b. (1990). saving the bathwater. child development, 60:1661-1671. brown, d.p. & taxman, s.i. (1993). five years of neonatal hearing screening: a summary. infant-toddler intervention: the transdisciplinary journal, 3(2):135-150. bu'lock, f., woolridge, m.w. & baum, j.d. (1990). development of co-ordination of sucking, swallowing and breathing: ultrasound study of term and preterm infants. developmental medicine and child neurology, 32:669-678. clarke-stewart, a. & friedman, s. (1987). child development: infancy through adolescence. new york: john wiley & sons. dunn, s.l., van kleeck, a. & rossetti, l.m. (1993). current roles and continuing needs of speech-language pathologists working in neonatal intensive care units. american journal of speechlanguage pathology, may:53-64. gordon, h.h. (1987). perspectives on neonatology. in avery, g b. (ed.) neonatology: pathophysiology and management of the newborn. philadelphia: j.b. lippincott. gorga, d., stern, f.m., ross, g. & nagler, w. (1988). neuromotor development of preterm and full-term infants. early human development, 18:137-149. jacobson, c.h. & shubat, s.j. (1991). hospital-based communication intervention with the at-risk newborn. infant-toddler intervention: the transdisciplinary journal, l(l):27-35. jaffe, m.b. (1989). feeding at-risk infants and toddlers. ibpics in language disorders, 10(l):13-25 keith, r.w. (1975). middle ear function in neonates. acta otolaryngologica, 101:376-3j79. klein, m.d. & briggs, m.h. (1987). facilitating mother-infant communicative interaction in mothers of high-risk infants. journal of childhood communication disorders, 10(2):95-106. kritzinger, a.m. (1994). vroee fcommunikasieontwikkeling van biologiese risikobabas. unpublished μ log thesis, university of pretoria. | lahey, m. (1988.) language disorders and language development. new york: macmillanj publishing company. louw, b. (1986). swart babas met gesplete .verhemelte: 'n morfofunksionele studie. unpublished d phil dissertation, university of pretoria. majnemer. α., brownstein, α., kadanoff, r. & shevell, m.i. (1992). a comparison of neurobehavioral performance of healthy term and low-risk preterm infants at term. developmental medicine and child neurology, 34:417-424. malan, b. (1993). spraak-taalterapeutiese/oudiologiese dienslewering aan 0-24 maande hoerisikokinders in die suidafrikaanse konteks. unpublished β log thesis, university of pretoria. mann, s.e. (1986). impedansoudiometrie by neonate. unpublished μ log thesis, university of pretoria. morris, s.e. & klein, m.d. (1987). prefeeding skills. tuscan: therapy skill builders. owens, r.e. (1984). language development. columbus: charles e. merrill publishing company. pearce, p.s., saunders, m.a., creigfaton, d.e. & sauve, r.s. (1988). hearing and verbal-cognitive abilities in high-risk preterm infants prone to otitis media with effusion. developmental and behavioral pediatrics, 9(6):346-351. piper, m.c., kunos, i., willis, d.m. & mazer, b. (1985). effect of gestational age on neurological functioning of the very lowbirthweight infant at 40 weeks. developmental medicine and child neurology, 27:596-605. proctor, a. (1989). stages of normal noncry development in infancy: a protocol for assessment. topics in language disorders, 10(l):26-42. proctor, l.r. & kennedy, d.w. (1990). high-risk newborns who fail hearing screening: implications for otological problems. seminars in hearing, 11(2):167-176. rosenblith, j.f. (1992). in the beginning: development from conception to age two. newbury park: sage publications. rossetti, l.m. (1990). infant-toddler assessment. an interdisciplinary approach. austin: pro-ed. rossetti, l.m. (1993). enhancing early intervention services to infants and toddlers and their families. journal of childhood communication disorders, 15(1): 1-6. salamy, a. & eldridge, l. (1991). neonatal risk and hearing loss. seminars in hearing, 12(2):146-153. smith, a. (1992). cuddling up like a joey in a pouch. sunday star, 20 september^. sparks, s.n. (1989). assessment and intervention with at-risk infants and toddlers: guidelines for the speech-language pathologist. ibpics in language disorders, 10(l):43-56. stjernqvist, k. & svenningsen, n.w. (1990). neurobehavioral development at term of extremely low-birthweight infants. developmental medicine and child. neurology, 32:679-688. touwen, b.c.l. (1990). variability and stereotypy of spontaneous motility as a predictor of neurological development of preterm infants. developmental medicine and child neurology, 32:501508. van kleeck, a. & richardson, a. (1988). language delay in the child. in lass, n.j. et al. (eds.) handbook of speech-language pathology and audiology. toronto: b.c. decker. walters, r.j. & shimizu, m.d. (1990). acoustical immittance screening of infants. seminars in hearing, ll(2):177-85. zarnoch, j.m. & balkany, t.j. (1977). tympanometry screening of normal and intensive care unit newborns: validity and reliability. in harford, e.r. et al. (eds.) impedance screening for middle ear disease in children. new york: grune & stratton. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) two s e p a r a t e a n d i d e n t i c a l c h a n n e l s t h r e s h o l d s p l o t t e d o n screen f r e q u e n c i e s 125 t o 20 000 hz w o r d l i s t s s t o r e d i n m e m o r y s o f t w a r e p a c k a g e 2 y e a r g u a r a n t e e i c u s t o m i z e d protocol's ι in p r i n t e r = p t y l t d t h e h e a r i n g h e a l t h c a r e s p e c i a l i s t s sovereign hous sovereign strei bedfordview 2008south africa p.o. box 630 bedfordview 2008 south africa tel: (011) 622-174 fax: (011) 622-130 reg. no. 77/01577/07 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 a m i w o o m [bir@alkodiig ftlh® s o u n d b a r r i e r ® r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) evalutaion of a programme.html evaluation of a programme to support foundation-phase teachers to facilitate literacy anna-marie wium department communication pathology , university of pretoria brenda louw professor emeritus: department communication pathology, university of pretoria irma eloff faculty of education, university of pretoria correspondence to: a wium (anna-marie1.wium@ul.ac.za) abstract learners who do not develop adequate listening and language skills during their early years are at risk of academic failure and early drop-out. future learning problems may be prevented by supporting these children in the foundation phase to overcome their developmental delays. a continued professional development (cpd) programme was developed to support foundation-phase teachers to facilitate literacy. the theoretical basis for the workshop material was the articulation between an auditory processing model, a language processing model, and literacy. the focus of this article is on the qualitative findings obtained from the literacy component of a more comprehensive cpd programme that covered several topics. the research was conducted as action research cycles across two contexts (a semi-rural and an urban-township context) and included 96 participants. this article explores how the teachers implemented the strategies to facilitate literacy in their classrooms and the benefits obtained from it. data were collected by means of questionnaires, self-reflections and focus groups, as well as a research diary and field notes. the results revealed that the strategies trained were implemented in the classrooms and were valued by the participants. those who participated in critical reflection felt that they had developed competence and professional growth. challenges identified included the language used in the support provided, which had an impact on phonological awareness training, and the use of terminology. the importance of collaboration was emphasised. the participants gained in the sense that they learnt how to implement the assessment standards in the curriculum, and learners benefited from the new strategies as they could all participate in the activities. the research confirmed the value of teacher support in the facilitation of literacy, which highlights the role of speech-language therapists working in school contexts. keywords: teacher support, listening, auditory processing, phonological awareness, qualitative research, speech-language pathologists background this study developed a continued professional development (cpd) programme for foundation-phase teachers to facilitate and promote emergent literacy skills. the first author acted as programme facilitator, and will be referred to as such throughout the article. the cpd programme was based on the principles of adult learning (knowles, holgotn, & swanson, 1998) and whole-brain learning (herrmann, 1996), to accommodate all learning preferences. the relationship between auditory processing and language processing information processing is a complex process (hamman & squire, 1996, 1997 in owens, 2004) that involves sensory input on many levels, which in turn is integrated and regulated by meta-cognition. it requires selective attention, inhibition, and the co-ordination of stimuli and concepts. the facilitation of literacy skills in this study is based on a three-level model that aligns a model for central auditory processing (bellis, 2003) with a language processing model (richards, 2004), which is then articulated with literacy. figure 1 was created to illustrate the link between bellis’s model for central auditory processing and how richards described the language processing model, and the learning outcomes for literacy (revised national curriculum statement (rncs)) (department of education, 2002). in figure 1, the first level of the central auditory processing model refers to how the sound signal is being received through the ear (bellis, 2002), which corresponds with the first level in the language processing model, described by richards (2004) as ‘listening skills’. listening is an active process that involves an awareness and localisation of sounds, as well as the behaviour (characteristics) of a good listener (bellis, 2003). the acquisition of such skills is an important first step in the processing of auditory input and also the first step in acquiring phonological awareness. learners need to learn the art of listening actively, attentively and analytically in order to learn (adams, foorman, lundberg, & beeler, 1998). the second level in the auditory processing model refers to the ‘signal manipulation’ level, which in turn corresponds with the ‘perception of speech’ (gillon, 2002) in the language processing model. this level includes both phonological awareness and phonemic processing. phonological awareness is critical to the ability to analyse (segment) speech units and to synthesise (blend) speech sounds into words, which makes it a strong predictor of success in reading and writing (goldsworthy, 1998; muter & diethelm, 2001). poor phonological awareness in turn negatively affects the acquisition of reading and spelling, so phonological awareness is viewed as the strongest predictor for academic success (ehri, nunes, willows, schuster, & yaghoub-z., 2001). learners need to develop phonological awareness skills to an age-appropriate level at school entry. many learners from low socio-economic schools (ses) have not developed adequate phonological awareness skills on entering school (nancollis, lawrie, & dodd, 2005). this may be attributed to limited or no prior literacy experiences at home. it is often found that such learners have limited access to structured preschool education. learners who are unable to read by the end of grade 1 tend to lag behind and may develop learning problems. on the third level of the auditory processing model (figure 1), the auditory signal is interpreted through higher cognitive functions, and relates to how meaning is extracted from the auditory input. richards (2004) considers the focus on this level to be more on linguistic skills than on auditory skills. such a view supports the notion that these two processes are closely related. bellis (2003: 95) is of the opinion that, ‘… it is not easy to separate acoustic and phonemic processing from one another or from higher-order linguistic influences’. to facilitate literacy development in the classroom, each of the three levels of language processing (richards, 2004) has a different effect on literacy learning (figure 1). the relationship between the three-level model and the national curriculum the national curriculum (nc) specifies ‘listening’ as the first learning outcome (lo1) for ‘literacy’ in the foundation phase (grades r 3). lo1 is a stepping stone for acquiring phonological awareness skills, which is integral to the development of emergent literacy skills. because the development of oral language is a prerequisite for the development of reading and writing (justice, meier, & walpole, 2005), this aspect is addressed in learning outcome 2 (lo2) of the rncs, (referred to as ‘speaking’) (figure 1). language skills include the visual modality (johnson & roseman, 2003), which is addressed in learning outcome 3 (lo3) (referred to as ‘reading and viewing’), as well as learning outcome 4 (lo4) (referred to as ‘writing’), where the focus turns towards acquiring more formal literacy skills. foundation-phase learners are also introduced to ‘thinking and reasoning’ in learning outcome (lo5) and ‘language structure and use’ in learning outcome 6 (lo6). the last four los correspond with the third level of the three-level language model, which is described as ‘linguistic skills’. the workshops provided in this cpd programme addressed each of the learning outcomes for literacy in the rncs, which again corresponds with the three levels of the model for language processing shown in figure 1. a cpd programme for foundation phase for the facilitation of literacy the content of this specific cpd programme was developed in collaboration with the gauteng department of education (provincial and district levels), and had to correlate with the rncs. the programme was considered a joint effort between the programme facilitator and the district facilitators. the other learning outcomes were addressed in the cpd programme as a whole, but this article focuses on the section that targeted the facilitation of ‘listening’ as a stepping stone for the development of phonological awareness and phonic awareness (refer to levels 1 and 2 in figure 1), as such skills are required for literacy. the cpd programme consisted of three components: a training component, supported by practical and mentoring components (wium, louw, & eloff, 2010). the training component in turn consisted of a series of workshops that were repeated in two contexts over a period of 2 years. the workshops (refer to appendix a) provided the participants with strategies and activities to facilitate literacy. the approach followed in the cpd programme was based on adult learning theories and made use of facilitative strategies for learning, e.g. action learning strategies (kemmis & mctaggart, 2005), co-operative learning (department of education, 2002; killen, 2007) and peer learning. experiential learning (kolb, 1984) opportunities allowed participants to practise the strategies through role play in the workshops and thereafter to implement it in their classrooms. such support allowed the participants to first observe the strategies before they were required to apply them, and then allowed them the opportunity to reflect on the process by completing self-reflection sheets for their portfolios. method aim of the research the aims of the article are to explore how the participants implemented the strategies to facilitate literacy in their classrooms, and to describe the benefits of the support provided. study design this study was part of a more comprehensive study using programme evaluation as research design (creswell & plano clark, 2007). the research made use of two action research cycles across two contexts (semi-rural and urban townships), and made use of qualitative methods of inquiry. participants the data collected for this article were obtained from the main study. in its effort to redress past inequalities, the gauteng department of education (gde) identified 24 low socio-economic schools (ses) in the tshwane region to participate in this project; 12 schools were from a semi-rural area, and 12 from an urban/densely populated area (including township schools and schools in informal settlements). stratified sampling was used to select the sample (mcmillan & schumacher, 2006), as each school that accepted the invitation to participate in the programme identified 1 teacher in each grade level of the foundation phase (e.g. grades r, 1, 2 and 3), so 4 teachers from each school enrolled for the programme, provided their participation was voluntary. there were 12 teachers representing each grade level in the foundation phase (grades r 3) included in the programme, totalling 96. at the time of the research it was estimated that there are about 3 4 classes in each grade level of each school, and therefore the selection of one participant from each grade level in each school represented approximately 25% of the total number of foundation-phase teachers in these selected schools. as only one primary trainer was available to conduct the workshops, groups of 48 participants per context were regarded as manageable, and were sufficient to allow for possible attrition later in the programme. in the larger study each group of four teachers in each school was encouraged to select a representative to attend the focus group meeting, which implies that these participants have already met the selection criteria for the original sample (nested design) (leech & onwuegbuzie, 2005). the focus groups consisted of 12 participants in each context (1 from each school), considered an adequate size for a focus group and a representative sample (25%) of the entire group that was trained. it also allowed for attrition (leedy & ormrod, 2010). focus groups were voluntary; in some instances more than one participant from each school attended the meeting, and in other cases none attended. the programme facilitator also acted as moderator in the focus groups, whereas the district facilitators acted as assistant moderators in both the contexts of the research. the district facilitators were required by the gde to assist the programme facilitator, and were partners in the project. both district facilitators were northern sotho speaking and familiar with research methods as they were both enrolled for master’s degrees at that time. with the exception of two participants, all were female. the sample was fairly homogeneous in terms of contexts, grade levels represented and the teachers’ experience in teaching, but not in terms of qualification, and therefore is considered as a realistic cross-section of the population (leedy & ormrod, 2005). the participants’ qualifications and prior learning may have been an advantage for some and a disadvantage for others, as the pace of training could have been too fast for some while adequate for others. questionnaire and self-reflection data were collected from 96 participants, whereas focus group data were contributed by 24 participants across the two contexts. in these specific contexts the most prominent language used as language of learning and teaching (lolt) was northern sotho (62%), followed by english (24%), setswana (8%) and isizulu (6%). all participants were part of the larger study and therefore were required to be appointed in full-time teaching positions in the foundation phase at schools in the targeted contexts. they also had to be willing to use english during the contact sessions, as it is the language used by the gde in all communication with and support of teachers. english is also the language used as medium of instruction at all institutions of higher education. this aspect was explained in the initial invitation letter to the schools, and also in the briefing meeting, so that participants could make informed decisions on whether they wanted to participate in the programme. participants who declined were not included. it was also emphasised that the teachers had to participate of their own free will and not as a result of coercion by their superiors. data collection qualitative data were collected from a variety of sources. all the participants from the main study attended the specific workshop to facilitate emergent literacy, and each of the 96 participants was expected to complete questionnaires after the workshops and to engage in self-reflection following a period of implementation of the strategies learnt in their classrooms. the original purpose of the questionnaires was mainly to collect quantitative data and therefore included only a limited number of open-ended questions, as they take longer to complete and therefore could be a cause of non-response (mcmillan & schumacher, 2010). such open-ended questions provided the opportunity for additional comments or recommendations, which was explanatory. all participants (n=96) were required to implement the strategies to facilitate literacy in their classroom following the workshops and to complete portfolio assignments for assessment. as part of the portfolio assignment they were required to engage in self-reflection (using reflection sheets) at the conclusion of the implementation period. such self-reflection is an inherent part of outcomes-based education (obe) (killen, 2007), and is known to facilitate deep learning. it was also a useful tool to monitor the implementation of the strategies. the two focus groups (each with 12 participants) were conducted 4 6 weeks after the workshops and were used to evaluate the workshop in terms of the participants’ perceived benefits, and to obtain feedback on their experiences in implementing the strategies. a focus-group schedule was used to guide the discussions. diary entries were made by the programme facilitator throughout the entire programme, without following any particular pattern. entries were made whenever the programme took a specific turn, or after a specific event took place, or when the researcher felt the need to reflect on specific issues. the aim of the research diary was to document the research process and to reflect on issues arising. it also provided insight regarding the system, and factors that could affect the outcomes of the programme. these entries were used to share ideas with experts and colleagues, and therefore elicited meta-reflection. all the workshop material and measuring instruments/procedures were developed in english, although particular examples were prepared in northern sotho for the facilitation of phonological awareness. it was acknowledged from the start that not all participants would be equally proficient in english, and because the programme facilitator had limited proficiency in the indigenous languages, arrangements were made with the district facilitators (each of whom was proficient in at least two african languages) to translate or interpret should the need arise. participants were encouraged to participate in their language of preference throughout the programme. credibility the credibility of the questionnaires was increased when a language editor reviewed and edited the questions. these questions were also scrutinised by two experts in the professional field, as well as a statistical advisor, to identify any potentially imprecise or ambiguous terms. pre-testing determined the clarity of instructions as well as questions, and the time for completion. focus group schedules were scrutinised by two experts prior to use to determine whether they would elicit the required responses. such measures increased the likelihood of trustworthiness. the programme facilitator acted as the moderator of the focus groups, and the district facilitators as assistant moderators, and as interpreters and translators when necessary. the district facilitators documented significant quotes and summarised each question discussed on the summary sheet specifically designed for this purpose. at the conclusion of the session, the district facilitators as assistant moderators verbally summarised the responses to questions. member-checking was done when these summaries were presented to the groups for approval, thereby increasing the trustworthiness of the data (bloor, frankland, thomas, & robson, 2001). the programme facilitator took field notes to supplement the summary and transcription of the audio recording. after the participants had departed, the programme facilitator (moderator) and the district facilitator (assistant moderator) met to reflect on the procedures, the participation, and outcomes of the session. they compared notes and confirmed the key ideas. shortly after the session the programme facilitator further reflected on the focus group by keeping a research diary. however, the fact that the assistant moderators were involved in the study may have biased the results to some extent. the audio recordings from the focus groups were transcribed verbatim by the course facilitator according to guidelines obtained from the literature (bloor, et al., 2001). coding was confirmed by 80% interand intra-rater agreement. for reasons of anonymity, speakers were referred to as ‘participant 1’, ‘participant 2’, etc. thick descriptions within the context were created and rich data from several data sources (diary entries, focus groups and open-ended questions) were obtained. it is acknowledged that the close proximity of the programme facilitator and the participants over time could have impacted on the results as the programme facilitator personally conducted the focus groups and transcribed, coded and analysed the data, and may have become subjective. data analyses the responses obtained from open-ended questions in questionnaires, as well as the self-reflections, were listed in word documents. the focus group sessions were transcribed verbatim and these, together with the self-reflections, diary entries and open-ended questions from questionnaires, were placed in a single hermeneutic unit and qualitatively analysed using content analyses. units were identified to answer the research questions (ryan & bernard, 2000) and were coded with the atlas-ti software suite (thomas muir scientific software development, 2003-2004), and categorised. the strength of atlas-ti is its ability to manage and organise large quantities of textual data. all text (apart from opening statements) was coded, and in turn categorised and grouped as major themes. the software used to analyse the qualitative data enabled the counting of specific codes (enumeration) to indicate the prominence of the various categories and themes. all items coded were categorised as either positive (confirming the research question) or negative (refute the research question) to provide a judgement in the evaluation of the programme, and were calculated as a percentage. results and discussion the findings were grouped as topics to answer the two research questions which relate to the implementation of strategies in the classroom, and the benefits obtained from the support provided. implementation of strategies in the classroom in response to the question: ‘how were the strategies implemented in the classroom?’ the following topics emerged. confirmation that strategies were applied in classrooms following the training the data firstly confirmed the implementation of strategies in the classrooms. the results showed that from the 125 items coded, 70% confirmed the implementation of strategies in classrooms. evidence of strategies being implemented in classrooms was obtained from portfolio assignments. strategies were implemented in the classrooms by using the lolt, which was in accordance with the language policy specified for the foundation phase (department of education, 2002). such results show a shift from what was the situation a decade ago, when the majority of teachers in gauteng were teaching in english (setati, adler, reed, & bapoo, 2003). mother-tongue or home-language instruction is considered most effective for learning in the foundation phase (motshekga, 2010). however, some participants acknowledged that the portfolio assignment was not a true reflection of their teaching as it was submitted without implementing the strategies. t: ‘there is no use to writing. you know writing, for the sake of a due date.’ (line 130, focus group 2(b)) a: ‘so some of you did the assignment without implementing it in the class. so you feel the assignment is not a true reflection of what is going on in the class? oh, ok.’ t: ‘but you … you don’t implement that what you have written on the assignment, you just write it to submit it to the lecturer. it is like studying for a degree.’ (line 200, focus group 2) such revelations indicated negative feelings (n=35), and because these individuals were from two specific schools, their attitudes could be school-related. a negative school culture has been identified as one of the reasons for dysfunctional schools (metcalfe, 2008). the participation in the cpd programme (e.g. implementation of strategies in the classroom as part of a portfolio assignment) depended on the participants’ motivation and attitudes, which emphasises the importance of including motivational strategies in future programmes. participants’ appreciation of the strategies the information included in the cpd programme for literacy was viewed positively as 73% (n=20) of the items coded as such indicated that the participants appreciated the information and the strategies taught. ‘i have learnt good ways of improving listening and be able to draw the attention of learners to listen attentively.’ (focus group 3(b)) ‘those strategies … we can now go on all day and forget about the time.’ (line 50, diary entry 29) the facilitation of listening requires teachers to firstly make learners aware of sound and to provide them with positive reinforcement for active attention to sound (bellis, 2003). such facilitation of strategies may imply a shift from the didactic approach where learners are instructed to listen, to a whole-body listening approach that focuses on active attending in class (bellis, 2002). critical reflection on practices/professional development currently reflective thinking in teacher support is emphasised as it facilitates quality teaching and professional development (cunningham, 2005). the participants reported that the implementation of strategies in their classrooms made them ‘think and reflect’ on their practices. as a result of the cpd programme several participants reported a change in their teaching practices. such reflection on practices is in keeping with the reflective competence required by the ‘norms and standards for teachers’ (department of education, 2000). reflection on their practices also put teachers in control of their own learning (bowles, 2004), which is in accordance with adult learning practices and therefore could be related to behaviour changes. ‘… improve my teaching, help me to reflect back’ (line 97, un-tabled open questions) ‘it makes you think.’ (line 217, focus group 1) ‘the workshop made a big difference to me because i could see that i was doing many wrong teaching in my teaching.’ (line 123, un-tabled open questions) however, the review of the portfolio assignments revealed that the personal reflection and self-assessments were often omitted. the fact that participants were required to complete the reflections by themselves in written format in the portfolios could have contributed to such omissions. it is also possible that the participants (and district facilitators) had little prior experience of reflective practices (killen, 2007) and did not know how to apply this technique. because of the recent introduction of these practices with the implementation of the obe approach (killen, 2007), the majority of the participants in this study may not have been trained in reflection and self-assessment. reflection is the basis for the successful implementation of obe (schwahn & spady, 1998). the participants’ inability to reflect on their own practices indicates that they had not yet mastered the basic skills required by an obe approach. reflection (from a technical or moral perspective) is an acquired skill that needs to be developed by practice and guidance (killen, 2007), and therefore this practice needs to be addressed in future programmes. challenges in the support provided language of delivery in the cpd programme a limitation of the workshop was that there were insufficient examples of phonological awareness in the different languages. despite preparing several examples in northern sotho, the participants required more impromptu examples in the workshops, and also in the other official african languages. some of the participants found it difficult to transfer the knowledge learnt in the workshop (in english) to the lolt used in their classrooms. despite having the district facilitators supporting the training, it proved challenging as the programme facilitator was not proficient in an african language and the district facilitators were not familiar with the concepts related to phonological awareness as they had not been pre-trained, and were also not proficient in other african languages. direct translation of english to the lolt is often not possible as it does not provide the required results (in many african languages a combination of words would be required to fully translate the meaning of a single english word). the multilingual south african context poses a challenge to speech-language therapists (slts) supporting teachers in training phonological awareness as currently less than 3% of slts have an african language as l1 (health professions council of south africa, 2005). a solution would be to have a teacher who is proficient in the lolt and who has a sound understanding of the underlying phonetic structure of the language as co-presenter of such workshops. concept of rhyming in african languages rhyming, as it appears in english, is a repetition of the final vowel-consonant cluster (johnson & roseman, 2003), (e.g. ‘the cat sat on the mat’), and is the first level in the development of phonological awareness (gillon, 2007). several comments (n=43) obtained from the data described the facilitation thereof as ‘difficult’, which implies that it is an unfamiliar concept in african languages (vermaak, 2006). ‘it was difficult for me, the rhyming. like, we don’t have so many rhymes like they have in english. so it was difficult with the lolt, to get like rhymes, to find rhymes. like we associate to do that. to get songs and rhymes. that was difficult for me.’ (line 205, focus group 1). these preliminary data call for critical consideration of facilitating rhyming as the first step in phonological awareness training in certain african languages, e.g. northern sotho, setswana and isizulu. if rhyming does not occur commonly in these african languages, then there is no point in training it as the concept cannot be explained to learners. further research is needed to determine the nature of rhyming in such languages. the question also arises whether this aspect should be facilitated in english additional language (eal) classrooms. examples obtained from portfolio assignments showed that the participants were more familiar with the concept of alliteration, which is repetition of a word beginning or ending with the same sound (e.g. ‘tloka, tlela’), with onset being the initial phoneme (johnson & roseman, 2003). the purpose of facilitating alliteration is similar to that of rhyming, in that it familiarises the ear to repetitive patterns of sound (at the beginning of words). would it then not be more suitable to focus on onset-rime in these african languages, as it is possible that the same benefits can be derived as for rhyming? this matter should be further researched. teachers’ unfamiliarity with new terminology the use of new terminology was, however, not generalised during the training as became evident when 64% (n=14) of the items were coded as ‘inability to recall the information’. p: ‘yeah, i think i benefited from it, because when i was trying this clapping method … so that the learners were enjoying it. they clapped two times, and then they clapped three times.’ a.m: yes – that was segmentation. yes … you will learn the terminology for these things soon … but i understand what you are saying. it was one of the strategies we did.’ (line 96, focus group 2). the above example indicates an awareness of specific concepts, which is the lowest level of acquiring new knowledge and thus regarded as ‘shallow learning’. such participants did not necessarily understand the information provided in the workshops, or know how to apply it. in several instances confusion in terminology was noted in the self-reflection in the portfolios, e.g. the term ‘auditory discrimination’ was used interchangeably with the term ‘rhyming’, as were ‘identification’ and ‘auditory memory’. this lack of understanding of these concepts became apparent early in the programme presented in the rural context. when the programme was repeated in an urban context the term ‘auditory discrimination’ was specifically emphasised and explained as: ‘... the difference between the sounds ...’ which appeared to be more effective, as no such confusion was noted again. sufficient repetition and explanation of new vocabulary is required in workshops, as discipline-specific terminology used by slts is unfamiliar to teachers. in a collaborative approach to providing teacher support it is necessary for slts and teachers to share their knowledge in order to come to a new understanding of such vocabulary in relation to the rncs. multidisciplinary collaboration should therefore be addressed in teacher preparation. benefits of the programme participants learnt to address assessment standards the results showed that the participants had previously omitted assessment standards in the curriculum because they did not know how to apply these. the participants believed that they had benefited from the training because they had learnt to address assessment standards in the rncs which they were unable to do before. ‘you know you helped us a lot. we used to skip most of the things.’ (line 284, focus group 1) strategies specified by the rncs to facilitate literacy, such as ‘riddles’ (used to facilitate auditory memory) and segmentation and blending activities, were particularly popular and were singled out by some participants as being successful and useful. ‘yes, in mother tongue i like the riddles, we also have the songs.’ (line 214, focus group 1) certain elements of phonological awareness were reportedly easy to teach in the lolt, specifically the segmentation of words as syllables and sounds, as well as the identification of the initial and final sounds of words. ‘… they specifically singled out “riddles” and “segmentation and blending activities” as being very effective and it seemed as if they have all implemented these strategies.’ (diary entry 14) many of the participants reported that they had previously omitted phonological awareness training from their curriculum because they did not understand the rationale thereof and did not know how to address it (even though it is specified in the rncs). adult learners learn more effectively when information is relevant to their needs and can be applied to their contexts (bowles, 2004). the participants were therefore more receptive to learn the new strategies, because as adult learners they were motivated to learn when they could understand the relevance of the learning objectives and activities for their own work (bowles, 2004). phonological awareness (in particular phonemic awareness) is facilitated in the context of literacy activities (lo2, lo3 and lo4). phonological awareness training in english (bernthal, bankson, & flipsen, 2009) follows a developmental sequence, of which rhyming is the first step in the english language (e.g. in nursery rhymes and songs, discrimination and production, e.g. ‘the cat sat on the mat’). this is followed by onset-rime, when the initial consonant changes the meaning and phonograms, e.g. ‘h-and’, ‘s-and’, ‘l-and’, ‘st-and’, etc. alliteration is repetition of a word beginning or ending with the same sound (e.g. ‘bana ba sekholo’). the next step is segmentation (auditory analyses), which is the ability to separate sentences as words; compound words, syllables, and also phonemes (e.g. b-u-s). segmentation of sounds consists of isolating initial, final, medial sounds (e.g. which sound is at the beginning/end or in the middle of ‘hat’?). it also comprises deletion of parts (e.g. say dustbin, say again without the ‘dust’ part). the most advanced levels are sound substitution (e.g. say ‘hat’, say it again but change the ‘h’ to ‘m’ = mat), and sound blending where sounds/components are connected in one meaningful utterance (e.g. ae-ro-plane = aeroplane, or sun + flower = sunflower). as mentioned previously, such skills require advanced knowledge of the sound system of the language, and therefore should ideally be facilitated by a teacher/facilitator who is proficient in the lolt. the facilitation of phonological awareness skills in the foundation-phase curriculum is a preventative strategy that enhances literacy development. it is of particular importance to learners from low ses, as they are at risk of experiencing difficulties in developing literacy skills (nancollis, et al., 2005). poor development of phonological awareness may lead to difficulty in reading and spelling (rvachew, chiang, & evans, 2007). reading and spelling problems can be prevented if phonological awareness is facilitated in the foundation phase, which justifies the inclusion of such information in teacher support programmes. benefits for learners participants in both contexts were exposed to information regarding phonological awareness and its role in facilitating literacy for the first time, and were excited about the effect the strategies had on their learners. t1: ‘... you know, we teachers have never done stories, songs and rhymes in class. we thought all of that in the rncs – it was for nothing. i feel our children ... their minds were caged in. we have since opened the screws, and the children came flying out like ... birds!’ (line 45, diary entry 16, focus group 1). the current study reported perceived gains made by learners, but these findings were subjective. research to determine the impact of programmes on learners’ performance is limited (khoza, 2007). recommendations for teacher support programmes phonological awareness should be presented by facilitators who are proficient in the lolt as the concepts cannot be translated directly as, for example, vocabulary. the support of teachers in the facilitation of phonological awareness in workshops firstly requires in-depth knowledge of the sound system of the lolt in order to generate language-specific examples. a clear understanding of the sound system of a language will allow programme facilitators to determine whether rhyming features in that language, and to plan an alternative means of facilitating where necessary. district facilitators who are proficient in african languages should be included in such workshops as co-presenters to facilitate phonological awareness skills. alternatively teachers who are proficient in the various african languages should be pre-trained as co-presenters of such skills. from the results obtained in this study it is proposed that research be conducted to determine whether alliteration rather than rhyming should be facilitated in schools where the lolt is an indigenous language. should research confirm this notion, it will infer a slight adaptation of the assessment criteria in the rncs. it is important for slts working in education contexts to acknowledge the uniqueness of the local language and culture (sowden, 2007). such sensitivity contributes to a better understanding of the specific dynamics embedded in the context, which may be attributed, at least in part, to the fact that south africa is a country characterised by considerable linguistic and cultural diversity. support programmes for teachers therefore cannot be generic in nature, but should be designed with consideration of the specific language and culture of the context. although such considerations may be time-consuming, they will be worthwhile to improve the performance of learners. slts have specific roles to play in education contexts. firstly, they have a preventive role, to provide preschool and foundation-phase teachers with support in the acquisition of literacy skills. secondly, they have to play a consultative and collaborative role in both district and school-based support teams to facilitate literacy and numeracy by providing training, mentoring, monitoring and consultation. it is recommended that district facilitators/teachers who are proficient in the lolt be included in the preparation of the workshop material, and also be pre-trained by the slt as co-presenters in such workshops. such measures build capacity and contribute to more effective collaboration. finally, it is important that such collaborative programmes be carefully documented as knowledge about their impact on learners’ performances is limited (khoza, 2007). the effect of cpd programmes for teachers on learners’ performance needs further investigation. conclusion the finding that the strategies trained through this specific cpd programme were mostly implemented in the lolt is in accordance with the language policy (department of education, 2002) for foundation-phase teaching and learning. such results show a shift from the situation a decade ago, despite the guidelines provided by the language policy at that time. the fact that the language policy is currently adhered to implies that progress has been made in the implementation of education policies, and that it is possible to change how teachers implement policy. the department of education has been effective in breaking down stereotyping and prejudices that existed with regard to english being considered by teachers and parents as superior to the local languages. in accordance with the language policy it is currently accepted throughout all levels (ranging through national, provincial, district and school levels) that mother-tongue or home-language instruction is considered as most effective for learning in the foundation phase (motshekga, 2010). it is disturbing to note that participants previously omitted phonological awareness because they did not understand this concept and did not have skills and strategies to teach it. this could have impacted on their learners’ development of literacy (justice & kaderavek, 2004). the support provided to the teachers in this study was considered effective as they felt that they could implement the strategies in their classrooms. such results also confirm that collaboration with district officials is important to achieve success, but that pre-training is required for optimal assistance in workshops. within a collaborative approach to teacher support, it is essential to establish positive and constructive relationships among slts, teachers and district facilitators, as this contributes to the success of adult learning experiences (galusha, 1998). it is therefore also essential that the education system supports slts in the execution of their tasks (law, 2002: 2, in o’toole & kirkpatrick, 2007). with regard to the slt’s role in supporting learners in the acquisition of literacy (department of education, 2001), it is imperative that teachers and slts work as a team, because as a team they can achieve so much more than when attempting anything on their own. acknowledgements . the authors wish to acknowledge the support of the shuttleworth foundation for supporting the fieldwork. references adams, m.j., foorman, b.r., lundberg, i., & beeler, t. (1998). phonemic awareness in young children: a classroom curriculum. baltimore, md: paul brookes publishing co. bellis, t. (2002). when the brain can’t hear: unravelling the mystery of auditory processing disorders. new york, ny: atria. bellis, t. (2003). assessment and management of central auditory processing disorders in the education setting: from science to practice (2nd ed.). clifton park, ny: thomson. bernthal, j.e., bankson, n.w., & flipsen, p. (2009). articulation and phonological disorders (6th ed.). boston, ma.: allyn & bacon. bloor, m., frankland, j., thomas, m., & robson, k. (2001). focus groups in social research. thousand oaks, ca: sage. bowles, t. (2004). adult approaches to learning and associated talents. australian journal of educational & developmental psychology, 4, 1-12. creswell, j.w., & plano clark, v.l. (2007). designing and conducting mixed methods research. thousand oaks, ca: sage. cunningham, b. (2005). mentoring teachers in post-compulsory education. london: david fulton publishers. department of education (2000). norms and standards for educators. department of education (2001). white paper 6: special needs education building an inclusive education and training system. pretoria. retrieved on 16 september 2011 from http://www.polity.org.za/govdocs/white_papers/educ6.html11 fr department of education (2002). revised national curriculum statement for schools: grades r 9. ehri, l.c., nunes, s.r., willows, s.r., schuster, b.v., & yaghoub z.s.t. (2001). phonemic awareness instruction helps children learn to read: evidence from the national reading panel’s meta-analysis. reading research quarterly, 36(3), 250-287. galusha, j.m. (1998). principles of training and of adult education: a comparison (no. ed416378). hattiesburg, ms: university of southern mississippi. gillon, g.t. (2002). phonological awareness intervention for children: from research laboratory to the clinic. asha leader, 7(22), 4-5, 16-17. gillon, g.t. (2007). phonological awareness: from research to practice. new york: guilford publishers. goldsworthy, c.l. (1998). sourcebook of phonological awareness activities. san diego, ca: singular publishing group. health professions council of south africa (2005). exit level outcomes. pretoria: hpcsa. herrmann, n. (1996). the whole brain business book. new york: mcgraw hill. johnson, k.l., & roseman, b.a. (2003). the source for phonological awareness. east moline, il: lingui systems. justice, l.m., & kaderavek, j.n. (2004). embedded-explicit emergent literacy intervention: background and description of approach. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 35, 201-212. justice, l.m., meier, j., & walpole, s. (2005). learning new words from storybooks: an efficacy study with at-risk kindergartners. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 36, 17-32. kemmis, s., & mctaggart, r. (2005). participatory action research. in n. k. denzin & y. lincoln (eds.), qualitative research (3rd ed.). london: sage. khoza, g. (2007, december 2007). a model for improving schooling. jet bulletin, 1-12. killen, r. (2007). teaching strategies for outcomes-based education (2nd ed.). cape town: juta. knowles, m.s., holgotn, e.f., & swanson, r.a. (1998). the adult learner: the definitive classic in adult education and human resource development. houston, tx: gulf publishing co. kolb, d.a. (1984). experiential learning: experience as the source of learning and development. new york, ny: prentice hall. leech, n.l., & onwuegbuzie, a.j. (2005). a typology of mixed methods research designs. paper presented at the the annual meeting of the american educational research association, montreal, canada. leedy, p.d., & ormrod, j.e. (2005). practical research: planning and design (5th ed.). upper saddle river, nj: pearson education. leedy, p.d., & ormrod, j.e. (2010). practical research (9th ed.). boston: pearson. lessing, a., & de wit, m.w. (2008, 30 september 1 october). do teachers know what the essential literacy skills are? paper presented at the laying solid foundations for learning conference, makopane, limpopo. mcmillan, j.h., & schumacher, s. (2006). research in education. boston, ma: allyn & bacon. mcmillan, j.h., & schumacher, s. (2010). research in education: evidence-based inquiry. boston: pearson. metcalfe, m. (2008, 13 january). why our schools don’t work ... and 10 tips how to fix them. sunday times, p. 10. motshekga, a. (2010, 6 july). statement by the minister of basic education, mrs angie motshekga, mp on the progress of the review of national curriculum statement. retrieved on 17 august 2011 from http://www.education.gov.za. muter, v., & diethelm, k. (2001). the contribution of phonological skills and letter knowledge to early reading development in a multilingual population. language learning, 51(2), 187-219. nancollis, a., lawrie, b.a., & dodd, b. (2005). phonological awareness intervention and the acquisition of literacy skills in children from deprived social backgrounds. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 36, 325-335. o’toole, c., & kirkpatrick, v. (2007). building collaboration between professionals in health and education through interdisciplinary training. child language teaching and therapy, 23(3), 325-352. owens, r.e. (2004). language disorders: a functional approach to assessment. boston, ma: allyn & bacon. richards, g. (2004). redefining auditory processing disorder: a speech-language pathologist’s perspective. asha leader, 9(6), 7-21. rvachew, s., chiang, p., & evans, n. (2007). characteristics of speech errors produced by children with and without delayed phonological awareness skills. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 38, 60-71. ryan, g., & bernard, r. (2000). data management and analysis methods. in n. k. denzin & y. lincoln (eds.), handbook of qualitative research (2 ed.). london: sage publications. schwahn, c., & spady, w.g. (1998). why change doesn’t happen and how to make sure it does. educational leadership (april), 45-47. setati, m., adler, j., reed, y., & bapoo, a. (2003). code-switching and other language practices in mathematics, science and english language classrooms in south africa. in j. adler & y. reed (eds.), challenges of teacher development: an investigation of take-up in south africa. pretoria: van schaik. sowden, p. (2007). culture and the good teacher in the english language classroom. elt journal, 61 (4), 304-310. thomas muir scientific software development (2003 2004). atlas-ti: the knowledge workbench v5.0. berlin, germany: thomas muir scientific software development. vella, j. (1994). learning to listen, learning to teach: the power of dialogue in educating adults. san francisco, ca: jossey-bass. vermaak, c. (2006). phonological awareness skills of a group of grade 4 learners, in a multi-cultural, multi-lingual education context with english as language of learning and teaching unpublished masters thesis, university of pretoria, pretoria. wium, a.m., louw, b., & eloff, i. (2010). speech-language therapists supporting foundation phase teachers with literacy and numeracy in a rural and township context. south african journal of communication disorders, 57(1), 14-22. fig. 1. the relationship between the three-levels of central auditory processing (bellis, 2003) with language processing (richards, 2004) and literacy outcomes (department of education, 2002). appendix a. content of the workshops to facilitate emergent literacy facilitating listening skills the participants in this study were provided with sufficient information to understand the rationale for facilitating listening skills, but also received strategies and opportunities to develop hands-on skills which allowed them to effectively facilitate listening skills. the participants were made to understand that in order for them to create an optimal listening environment in their classroom, may require of them to make some acoustic and teacher-based environmental modifications (bellis, 2003). the programme made participants aware of how to minimise interfering factors (goldsworthy, 1998) and how to facilitate listening behaviour that facilitates auditory attention (e.g. whole-body listening strategies) (bellis, 2003). furthermore, the workshops included strategies and activities to facilitate auditory tasks, e.g. auditory discrimination, memory, sequencing, figure-ground and perception of speech, which are required for language development, but also for phonological processing skills. facilitating phonological processing several teachers in the current education system feel unsure about the facilitation of phonological awareness and have a need for support. less than 5% of the teachers in lessing and de wit’s (2008) study in mpumalanga and limpopo provinces reported that they had confidence in teaching the sub-skills for literacy acquisition. this may be attributed to the fact that the role of phonological awareness in the development of literacy only became fully known in the early 1990s and therefore was not included in the professional training of teachers until much later. the facilitation of emergent literacy skills have been included in this cpd programme because of its relevance to literacy learning, but also to address a need of teachers who had not been trained in this aspect before. the cpd programme addressed the skills required to develop phonological awareness, e.g. rhyming, alliteration, segmentation, sound blending, and sound manipulation (gillon, 2002, 2007; goldsworthy, 1998). facilitation of phonological awareness starts with rhyming songs and nursery rhymes, and then proceeds to make the learners aware of words in a sentence (e.g. i-sit-on-a-chair), followed by awareness of syllables (e.g. but-ter-fly). lastly, the focus is on the awareness of sounds (phonemes) which ultimately results in blending and segmenting individual phonemes (e.g. j-u-m-p; c-a-t, rhi-no-ce-ros) (bernthal, et al., 2009). in addition, skills such as auditory closure, auditory association, and phonemic analysis linked to phoneme identification, grapheme-phoneme identification, and grapheme-phoneme correspondence were also included (richards, 2004). the workshop activities included demonstrations and practice of the identification of initial sounds, end-sounds, the segmentation of sentences into words, words into syllables, and individual sounds. blending of syllables and sounds, as well as sound manipulation was also addressed. although the inclusion of songs and rhymes in the facilitation of literacy is a good start towards the development of phonological awareness, the traditional actions that accompany these activities are intended to facilitate the meanings of words and not necessarily to focus on the sound structure of the language. participants were made aware that it is necessary to import different strategies into their classroom practices, e.g. waving hands when rhymes are heard, clapping hands/stomping feet when alliteration patterns are recognised, clapping the syllables in peers’ names, and slowly stretching of arms when syllables are blended to form words. 67 early detection of ototoxicity by high-frequency audiometry — a case study gustav r. voogt, ba(log) (pretoria) department of otorhinolaryngology, medical university of southern africa. abstract the effect of an ototoxic aminoglycoside antibiotic jtobramycinj on the hearing acuity of an adult black female cardiac patient was evaluated with a new type of high-frequency audiometer. results indicated the effectivity of this audiometer for the early detection of ototoxicity. the possibility of higher susceptibility to ototoxic damage in blacks due to a higher concentration of melanin in the inner ear is discussed. opsomming die effek van 'n ototoksiese aminoglikosied antibiotikum (tobramycin) op die gehoorsensiwiteit van 'n volwasse swart vroulike hartpasient is geevalueer met behulp van 'n nuwe tipe hoefrekwensieoudiometer. resultate dui op die effektiwiteit van hierdie oudiometer vir die vroee opsporing van ototoksisiteit. die moontlikheid van 'n hoer vatbaarheid vir otoksiese skade by swartes as gevolg van 'n hoer konsentrasie van melamien in die binneoor word bespreek. conventional diagnostic audiometry generally deals with assessment of auditory sensitivity for frequencies of 8 khz and below, even though humans can hear tones as high as 16 to 20 khz. it has also been proven that ototoxic substances first cause a decline in the high-frequency hearing, i.e., above 10 khz (schuknecht, 1974). various drugs are known to be ototoxic. of these, tobramycin is rated a?highly ototoxic (lane and routledge, 1983). in these reports the ototoxic effects were determined by histologic examinations of the hair cell damage occurring in the cochlea and by measuring thfe resultant decline in hearing sensitivity as measured by conventional pure tone audiometry (fee, 1980; smith, lipsky, laskin, hellmann, mellits, longstreth and lietman, 1980; matz, 1986). ototoxic agents cause hair cells to begin to degenerate first at the very basal end of the organ of corti, that part of the cochlea which is used to detect the highest frequencies the living animal can hear. this is a process that gradually and systematically progresses farther into the cochlea (schuknecht, 1974). by the time this damaging effect becomes visible on a conventional pure tone audiogram valuable time for prevention has passed and permanent damage has been done to the high frequency region in the cochlea. thus, by measuring high-frequency hearing, drug-induced ototoxic damage can be detected at a much earlier stage (tonndorf and kurman, 1984). this preoccupation with testing may be explained by various mechanoacoustic problems encountered when trying to test for the higher frequency auditory thresholds. the biggest problem appears to be in calibration as the quarter and half wavelengths of the higher frequency sound approach ear canal width and length, causing transversal resonances and standing waves to occur. this means that the sound front impinging on the tympanic membrane no longer resembles the sound being fed into the external ear canal (stinson, 1984; tonndorf and kurman, 1984). logicaldie suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34,1987 ly this also leads to immense problems when trying to mask the contralateral ear. a further problem seems to be to find audiometers and transducers capable of producing these high-frequency stimuli at the necessary amplitude and fidelity but still maintaining a reasonably flat response curve. then there are also a vast number of smaller aspects which still need clarification, e.g., high-frequency interaural attenuation, inter subject variability in responding to highfrequency sounds, etc. quite a number of large-scale studies of high-frequency hearing can be found in the literature (rosen, plester, el-mofty, and rosen, 1964; zislis and fletcher, 1966; harris and myers, 1971; northern, downs, rudmose, glorig and fletcher, 1971). it is, however, practically impossible to compare results due to differences in audiometer, transducer, ear coupling, calibration methods and equipment, testing method and environment, population characteristics, selection criteria and very high interand intra-individual variability. as such, no normative audiometric threshold values exist for high-frequency audiometry (fausti, frey, erickson and rappaport, 1979; fletcher, 1965; de seta, bertoli and filipo, 1985; gauz and smith, 1985; henry, east, nguyen, paolinelli and ayors, 1985). furthermore it has been found that certain drugs like the polycyclic amines, especially the aminoglycocide antibiotic tobramycin, has a very high melanin affinity (potts, 1962a; 1962b; 1964a; 1964b; potts and au, 1971; 1976; lindquist, 1973). add to this the fact that melanin is present in quite large quantities in the cochlea, as first reported by the italian anatomist alfonso corti as far back as 1851. this may be an important factor in the etiology of drug induced ototoxicity (dencker and lindquist, 1975; dencker, lindquist and ullberg, 1975; wasterstrom, 1984; wasterstrom, brendberg, lindquist, lyttkens and rask-anderson, 1986). due to the earlier-mentioned problems encountered when trying to measure high-frequency thresholds, a new type of © sasha 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early detection of ototoxicity by high-frequency audiometry — a case study 68 high-frequency audiometer, the tonndorf audimax model 500, has recently been developed which could possibly bypass most of these problems (tonndorf and kurman, 1984). this audiometer works on the principle of electrostimulation. the test signal is superimposed on a modulated carrier frequency and is delivered via mylar-coated electrodes into the skin over each mastoid (fig. 1.). as the subject is then capacitatively coupled to the electrodes, no real current flows between the electrodes and the subject. numerous studies have identified electrostimulation as a order to guard against possible renal failure. any renal failure during this period would have resulted in very high serum levels due to the accumulation of tobramycin in the bloodstream. the hearing test battery as mentioned above was repeated postoperatively on day 3, day 6, day 13 and day 20. on postoperation (po) day 21 she was discharged from hospital. three months later when she came to hospital for a followup examination her hearing was tested again according to r f carrier (60 khz) audiofrequency (0.2-20khz) modulator 7 att / l τ mylarcoated electrodes •s^mpl fig 1: schematic of high-frequency audiometer (from: tonndorf and kurman, 1984) means of audio-transmission of electromechanical vibration in the bone and tissue structures surrounding the inner ear and the cochlea. thus it would appear that the subject's bone-conduction hearing is being tested (sommers and von gierke, 1964). this audiometer tests frequencies from 200 hz right up to 20 khz in 200 hz steps. the stimulus intensities can be adjusted from 0 to 120 electrostimulation hearing threshold levels (eshtl) in 1 eshtl step sizes. zero to 120 eshtl corresponds with zero to 60 db spl. it was therefore decided to use this new audiometer to monitor the very early ototoxic effect of tobramycin on the high-frequency hearing of a black cardiac surgery patient in ga-rankuwa hospital, near pretoria. methodology the subject for this case report is a 25 year old black woman who required open-heart surgery. she had to use tobramycin prophylactically for a period starting immediately after the operation. before the operation her hearing was tested on two consecutive days by impedance audiometry, conventional pure tone audiometry and high-frequency audiometry. seeing that both ears tested almost identically the test results for left and right ears were combined. as no standardized norms for high-frequency thresholds exist, it was decided to use the first two high-frequency test results as the biological baseline. on the day of the operation she received 40 mg tobramycin and thereafter the consecutive doses were altered so that the "tobramycin serum levels were maintained between a trough of not less than 2 fig/ml and a peak not exceeding 10 mg/ml. this range is considered ototoxically safe (matz, 1986). the tobramycin regimen was continued for 72 hours. throughout this period, the tobramycin blood serum levels were carefully monitored, as well as the renal functioning, in the above test battery. the initial two tests before the operation served to check on test-retest reliability and also served as the control test against which all further test results were to be compared. results on impedance audiometry no differences could be detected for consecutive tests except for very small variations in middle-ear pressure. all the other impedance test results (maximum compliance, acoustic reflex thresholds) remained essentially the same throughout the test period. ! conventional pure tone audiometry test results also remained essentially unchanged, with only about 5 db total differences in thresholds between tests. j high-frequency audiometry, on the other hand, showed marked changes (fig. 2). the first two pre-operation test' results were decidedly identical, indicating very good test-1 retest reliability. on p.o day 3 there was a very clear' decrease in high-frequency sensitivity, especially at the high end of the hearing range. this decrease continued as is shown on the po day 6 test. on po day 13 there was a marked recovery, but not back to the original pre-operation hearing levels. the last test before the patient was discharged on po day 20 indicated that the recovery process had ceased and the test results were similar to those of po day 13. the follow-up test done 3 months after discharge from hospital matched that of po days 13 and 20, thus indicating that there was no further recovery whatsoever. it is to be noted that this patient did not suffer from any renal dysfunction during the entire period. thus there was no possibility of tobramycin accumulation due to renal failure which could have led to the tobramycin exceeding the ototoxically safe upper serum level of 10 fig/ml. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gustav r. voogt 69 0 5 10 15 20 25 30 35 4 0 45 5 0 5 5 60 65 70 75 80 85 90 95 100 105 110 115 120 pre-op test 1 • pre-op test 2 3 d a y po 6 -day po 1 3 d a y po 20-day po 9 0 d a y po 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 frequency (khz) fig 2: high-frequency threshold changes over time discussion it would appear that high-frequency audiometry by electrostimulation shows very good test-retest reliability, seeing that the average difference in thresholds between the first two tests was no larger than 5 eshtl units, corresponding to about 2,0 to 2,5 db spl. high-frequency audiometry could detect ototoxicity at a very early stage, as well as the severity of damage and recovery from damage, whilst standard audiometry and impedance tests were still showing no effects whatsoever of ototoxicity on hearing sensitivity. ί it is clear from fig. 2 that the otoxic effect continued long after cessation of drug administration and long after the serum levels indicated no drug residue in the bloodstream. this might be further proof of the possible accumulation of this drug on the melanin ofj the inner ear. this accumulation could, however, not be proved in this case study as no biopsies could be taken from or histological examinations done on the patient's inner!ear. on the other hand, if the drug really accumulates in |the inner ear, the monitoring of drug/serum levels would be of no use, since it would not represent the actual level of tobramycin in the inner ears. it may also be possible that blacks have more melanin in the inner ear than whites (dencker and lindquist, 1975; dencker et al. 1975) and are thus more susceptible to ototoxic damage, but this is only speculative as no human experimental data on this aspect could be found in the literature. this possibility has, however, been proven in a study done on albino and pigmented guinea pigs (wasterstrom, 1984; wasterstrom et al. 1986). thus it would appear that the "safe" serum levels for ototoxic drugs were based on hearing tests for frequencies up to 8 khz only and may really not be safe at all. it would appear that the monitoring of patients for drug-induced ototoxicity should rather be done by high-frequency audiometry or a combination of measuring blood/serum levels and high-frequency audiometry. conclusion high-frequency electrostimulation audiometry seems to show good test-retest reliability. it is also very effective in the early detection of ototoxicity, compared to standard audiometry. ototoxic drug accumulation, possibly on the melanin in the inner ear will have to be investigated further. also the possibility of blacks having more melanin in the inner ear and thus a higher susceptibility to drug-induced ototoxicity needs further research. lastly, a thorough investigation of present ototoxically "safe" serum levels is required. acknowledgements the loan of the tonndorf audimax 500 high-frequency audiometer by the needier westdene organization pty ltd, johannesburg is acknowledged with thanks. references dencker, l. and lindquist, n.g. distribution of labelled chloroquinine in the inner ear. arch. otolaryngol, 101, 185-188, 1975. dencker, l., lindquist, g.g. and ullberg, s. distribution of 125i-labelled chloroquinine analogue in a pregnant macaca monkey. toxicology, 5, 255-265, 1975. de seta, e., bertoli, g.a. and filipo, r. high frequency audiometry above 8 khz. audiology, 24, 254-259, 1985. fausti, s.a., frey, r.h., erickson, d.a. and rappaport, b.z. 2afc versus standard clinical measurement of high frequency auditory sensitivity (8-20 kc/s)./. aud. res., 19, 151-157, 1979. fee, w.e. aminoglycoside ototixicity in the human. laryngoscope, 90, (suppl 24), 1-18, 1980. fletcher, j.l. reliability of high frequency thresholds. j. aud. res., 5, 133-137, 1965. gauz, m.t. and smith, m.m. the simplified hf e-800 audiometer : calibration and normative aspects. j. aud. res., 25, 101-122, 1985. harris, j.d. and myers, c.k. tentative audiometric hearing threshold level standards from 8 through 18 kilohertz./. acoust. soc. am., 49, 600-601, 1971. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 early detection of ototoxicity by high-frequency audiometry — a case study henry, k.r., east, g.a., nguyen, h.h., paolinelli, m.c. and ayors, n.m. extra-high-frequency auditory thresholds : fine structure, reliability, temporal integration and relation to ear canal resonance. audio!., 24, 92-103, 1985. lane, r.j.m. and routledge, p.a. drug-induced ototoxicity. drugs, 26, 124-147, 1983 lindquist, n.g. accumulation of drugs on melanin. acta radiol (stockhj suppl. 325, 1-92, 1973. matz, g.j. aminoglycoside ototoxicity, am. j. otolaryngol., 7, 117-119, 1986. northern, j.l., downs, m.p. rudmose, w., glorig, a. and fletcher, j.l. recommended high-frequency audiometric threshold levels (8000-18000 hz], j. acoust. soc. am., 52, 585-597, 1971. potts, a.m. the concentration of phenothiazines in the eye of experimental animals. invest. opthalmol, 1, 522-530, 1962a. potts, a.m. uveal pigments and phenothiazine compounds, tr. am. opthalmol soc., 60, 517-552, 1962b. potts, a.m. further studies concerning the accumulation of polycyclic compounds on uveal melanin. invest. opthalmol, 3, 399-404, 1964a. potts, a.m. the reaction of uveal pigment in vitro with polycyclic compounds. invest. opthalmol, 3, 405-416, 1964b. potts, a.m. and au, p.c. thallous ion and the eye. invest. opthalmol, 10, 925-931, 1971. potts, a.m. and au, p.c. the affinity of melanin for inorganic ions. exp. eye res., 22, 487-491, 1976. rosen, s., plester, d., el-mofty, a. and rosen, h. high frequency audiometry in presbycusis. arch. otolaryngol, 79, 25-35, 1964. schuknecht, h.f. pathology of the ear. cambridge, massachusets: harvard university press, 1974. smith, c.r., lipsky, j.j., laskin, o.l., hellman, d.b., mellits, e.d., longstreth, j. and lietman, p.s. double blind comparison of the nephrotoxicity and auditory toxicity of gentamycin and tobramycin. n. engl j. med., 302, 1106-1108, 1980. sommers, h.c. and von gierke, h.e. hearing sensations in electric fields. aerospace med., 35, 834-839, 1964. stinson, m.r. audiometry and the geometry of the human ear canal caa symposium, quebec city, 1984. tonndorf, j. and kurman, b. high frequency audiometry. ann. otol rhinol. laryngol., 93, 576-582, 1984 wasterstrom, s. accumulation of drugs on inner ear melanin: therapeutic and ototoxic mechanisms. scand. audiol. suppl. 23, 1-40, 1984. wasterstrom, s., bredberg, g., lindquist, n.g., lyttkens, l. and rask-andersen, h. ototoxicity of kanamycin in albino and pigmented guinea pigs i: a morphologic and electrophysiologic study. am. j. otol, 7, 11-18, 1986. zislis, t. and fletcher, j.l. relation of high-frequency thresholds to age and sex./ aud. res., 6, 189-198, 1966. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) •ί*· ί y new automatic hearing aid a truly advanced and innovative sound amplifier the automatic sound processor (asp) was developed especially for those people who have difficulty in hearing speech in noisy environments. for years, hearing aid wearers have avoided restaurants, theatres, church and parties because of the inability of an ordinary hearing aid to control background noises. the a.s.p. is simply and uniquely designed to normalize background noise so that speech is more easily understood. to be able to restore sound to even one human being makes your life especially important. 4l 7, , , ^ τ l. to restore hearing... what a blessed talent to have medifix "earing system co. surgical & medical p.o. box 19 bedfordview 2008 south africa phone: (011)53-4188/9 cape p.o. box 52 lynedoch 7603 phone: (02234) 442 or (02231) 93442 natal p.o. box 47443 greyville 4023 phone: (031)236164 m r i c h a r d s hearing systems tomorrow's technology for today's hearing problem r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 information for contributors the south african journal of communication disorders publishes reports and papers concerned with research, or critically evaluative theoretical, or therapeutic issues dealing with disorders of speech, voice, hearing or language, or on aspects of the processes underlying these. the south african journal of communication disorders will not accept material which has been published elsewhere or that is currently under review by other publications. all contributions are reviewed by at least two consultants who are not provided with author identification. form of manuscript. authors should submit four neatly typewritten manuscripts in triple spacing with wide margins which should not exceed much more than 25 pages. each page should be numbered. the first page of two copies should contain the title of the article, name of author/s, highest degree and address or institutional affiliation. the first page of the remaining two copies should contain only the title of the article. the second page of all copies should contain only an abstract (100 words) which should be provided in both english and afrikaans. afrikaans abstracts will be provided for overseas contributors. all paragraphs should start at the left margin and not be indented. major headings, where applicable, should be in the order of method, results, discussion, conclusion, acknowledgements and references. tables and figures should be prepared on separate sheets (one per table/figure). figures, graphs and line drawings must be originals, in black ink on good quality white paper. lettering appearing on these should be uniform and professionally done, bearing in mind that such lettering should be legible after a 50% reduction in printing. on no account should lettering be typewritten on the illustration. any explanation or legend should not be included in the illustration but should appear below it. the titles of tables and figures should be concise but explanatory. the title of tables appears above, and of figures below. tables and figures should be numbered in order of appearance (with arabic numerals). the amount of tabular and illustrative material allowed will be at the discretion of the editor (usually not more than 6). references. references should be cited in the text by surname of the author and date, e.g. van riper (1971). where there are more than two authors, et al. after the first author will suffice. the names of all authors should appear in the reference list. references should be listed alphabetically in triple-spacing at the end of the article. for acceptable abbreviations of names of journals, consult the fourth issue (october) of dsh abstra cts or the world list of scientific periodicals. the number of references used should not exceed much more than 20. note the following examples: locke, j.l. clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341, 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed̂ handbook of clinical audiology, 3rd ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, 1971. proofs. galley proofs will be sent to the author wherever possible. corrections other than typographical errors will be charged to the author. reprints. 10 reprints without covers will be provided free of charge. all manuscripts and correspondence should be addressed to: the editor, south african journal of communication disorders, south african speech and hearing association, p.o. box 31782, braamfontein 2017, south africa. inligting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings publiseer verslae en artikels oor navorsing, of krities evaluerende artikels oor die teoretiese of terapeutiese aspekte van spraak-, stem-, gehoorof taalafwykings, of oor aspekte van die prosesse onderliggend aan hierdie afwykings. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings sal nie materiaal aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. alle bydraes word deur minstens twee konsultante nagegaan wat nie ingelig is oor die identiteit van die skrywer nie. formaat van die manuskrip. skrywers moet vier netjies getikte manuskripte in 3-spasi'ering en met bree kantlyn indien, en dit moet nie veel langer as 25 bladsye wees nie. elke bladsy moet genommer wees. op die eerste bladsy van 2 afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, die naam van die skrywer/s, die hoogste graad behaal en die adres of naam van hulle betrokke instansie verskyn. op die eerste bladsy van die oorblywende twee afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskyn. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. alle paragrawe moet teenaan die linkerkantlyn begin word en moet nie ingekeep word nie. hoofopskrifte moet, waar dit van toepassing is, in die volgende volgorde wees: metode, resultate, bespreking, gevolgtrekking, erkennings en verwysings. tabelle en figure moet op afsonderlike bladsye verskyn (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie). figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge moet oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte gedoen word. letterwerk wat hierop verskyn moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50%-verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by die illustrasie moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verklarings of omskrywings moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maar daaronder verskyn. die byskrifte van tabelle moet bo-aan verskyn en die van figure onderaan. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word (met arabiese syfers). die hoeveelheid materiaal in die vorm van tabelle en illustrasies wat toegelaat word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonlik nie meer as 6 nie). , verwysings. verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, bv. van riper(1971). waar daar meer as twee skrywers is, sal et al. na die eerste skrywer voldoende wees. die name van alle skrywers moet in die verwysingslys verskyn. verwysings moet alfabeties in 3-spasi'ering aan die einde van die artikel gerangskik word. vir die aanvaarde afkortings van tydskrifte se titels, raadpleeg die vierde uitgawe (oktober.) van dsh abstracts of the world list of scientific periodicals. die getal verwysings wat gebruik is, moet nie veel meer as 20 wees nie. ' let op die volgende voorbeelde: ' locke, j.l. clinical phonology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders. j. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341 1983. penrod, j.p. speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.)handbook of clinical audiology, 3de ed., baltimore: williams & wilkins, 1985. van riper, c. the nature of stuttering. eng|ewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice hall, 1971. proewe. galeiproewe sal waar moontlik aan die skrywec-gestuur' word. die onkoste van veranderings, behalwe tipografiese foute, sal deur die skrywer self gedra moet word. herdrukke. 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis verskaf word. alle manuskripte en korrespondensie moet gerig word aan: die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings. die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraaken gehoorheelkunde, posbus 31782, braamfontein 2017, suid-afrika. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the pragmatics of language: theoretical and applied issues carol a prutting phd university of california santa barbara, california abstract the paper reviews the contributions made by the paradigm shift which has occurred in our discipline. comments arc also made from the broader perspective of the philosophy of science. opsomming die artikel beskou die bydrae wat gemaak is deur die paradigmatiese verskuiwing watplaasgevind het in ons dissipline. opmerkings word ook gemaak vanuit 'n breer perspektief van die filosofie van die wetenskap. "i cannot give any scientist of any age better advice than this: the intensity of the conviction that a hypothesis is true has no bearing on whether it is true or not" (medawar, 1979, p. 39). once a hypothesis is made the scientist is in business. it guides the observation and suggests empirical tests which might otherwise not have been performed. in the past few years a shift in out theoretical models has taken place. we, as a discipline, have moved from a linguistic paradigm (chomsky, 1957; 1965) to a philosophical one (austin, 1962; searle, 1969). the,changes we have experienced and what this shift represents for the field of speech and language pathology will be discussed. under the linguistic generative grammar paradigm the innate predisposition to learn language was of primary import for learning an abstract set of rules to account for one's knowledge of language. in the philosophical speech act paradigm an account of what speakers and listeners do in various communicative interactions is of paramount interest. within this framework much attention is given to the contextual influences in the environment which account for one's ability to perform in a competent manner. in the case of the former model the focus is on the speaker. in the latter model, the unit of analysis is the dyad. proponents of the linguistic paradigm define language as a set of sentences. the philosophical paradigm defines, language as an instrument for social interaction. generative grammarians view the function of language as the expression of thought, whereas for the speech act enthusiasts, the function is to communicate. competence is viewed differently as well for both camps. from a linguistic framework competence is discussed as the ability to produce, comprehend and judge grammaticalness. in contrast, from a philosophical viewpoint competence is rooted in social competence, i.e. the ability to initiate, maintain, and terminate relationships. humboldt (1836), writing over a century ago, inspired chomsky (1957, 1965) with his notion of the generative aspects of language. humboldt maintained that language makes infinite use of finite means. austin (1967) and searle (1969) were influenced by the early pragmatist peirce (1878) as well as james (1907) and wittgenstein (1958) all of whom believed that language meaning was in its use. peirce, in turn, was very much affected by the early philosophy of kant (1781). it is true that rival theories such as the linguistic and speech act seldom address the exact same set of problems. some issues are lost and others gained whenever different theories evolve. kuhn (1962) suggested that no paradigm solves all problems. however, paradigm debates do revolve around the question: which particular problems are more significant to have solved? there is a certain discomfort which takes place when theories shift within a discipline. the discomfort can be replaced by the quest for solutions, a respect for time, and an understanding that in science there are more disproofs than there are proofs. lewis thomas (1983), recent author of the youngest science, suggests a healthy state is that of "informed bewilderment" with our feet firmly planted in mid-air. this is a difficult state to maintain especially among the younger members of a profession. theories are invaluable in that they can be likened to road maps for problem solving. they lead us to ask certain questions and select one methodological solution over another. our empiricism does not always keep pace with our theoretical constructs. however, methodology can sometimes outstrip theory. for instance, both the babylonians and egyptians employed * this paper is a written version of the ρ de v pienaar memorial lecture delivered on september 21, 1983 at the university of pretoria. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 'v sash a 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 carol a prutting mythology to explain astronomical phenomena. for the babylonians the earth was a hollow mountain and the universe was an oyster with water underneath and overhead. similarly, the egyptians conceived of the universe as a more rectangularly shaped oyster or box; the earth was its floor, while the sky was a cow whose feet rested on the four corners of the earth. during this time chaldean priests observed the stars and made maps and timetables of positions and movements. the timetables became calendars used to regulate agricultural activities and religious ceremonies. these observations and calculations were precise and deviated less than .001 % from our modern calculations. in this example we have an exact science, in terms of accurate predictions, outstripping a theoretical construct. gould (1981), in an excellent book entitled the mismeasure of man, writes about science as a social act which has subjectivity at its core. science is that exploratory activity whose purpose is to understand the world for the betterment of society. the actual exploratory activity is termed research. there are many theories generated and many ways of going about understanding and bringing order to the phenomena under study. medawar (1979) has discussed the various ways toward understanding: baconian — truth is all around us and one needs only to observe things as they really are. aristotelian — one has preconceived notions of the world and one needs to act on these ideas. galilean — one discriminates between different ways of doing something and this either gives one confidence in one's view or alters one's view in some way. kantian — one's view of the world is based on a prior knowledge. all of these approaches are justifiable and each, in somewhat different ways, adds to and contributes to our understanding of the world. once our theoretical underpinnings are tightened they give way to methodological considerations. some of our choices in design are descriptive, experimental, longitudinal, and cross-sectional. the choice one makes generally has to do with ones theoretical orientation, the research question asked, and the background of literature in which the study will be embedded. the theoretical shift which has taken place in our discipline is enumerated elsewhere (prutting, 1982). for the purposes of this paper only the sediments which remain after shifting through the speech act theory will be presented. the implications of these changes are far reaching for the communicatively disordered client. what follows is an outline of the changes as a result of our theoretical shift: assessment principles areas of assessment: analysis across linguistic, cognitive, and social domains. bates (1979) slightly tongue and cheek concludes there are some 30 000 possible models for intervention principles competence: goals should be toward relevant and functional communicative interaction. the demonstration of understanding this threeway relationship. as fuller (1975) suggests with his idea of "synergy" — the behavior of whole systems is unpredicted by their parts taken separately. it is therefore important to view the three areas in relationship to the entire communicative system of any given client. unit of measurement: the dyad should be the unit of analysis since competence lies in the relational system and as such should be assessed with relational parameters intact. societal as priority over clinical judgements: societal judgements are concerned with appropriate or inappropriate appraisals rather than correct/incorrect. a behavior may indeed be incorrect but appropriate. appropriate is defined as positive or neutral effects on the interaction while inappropriate behaviours detract or penalize the communicator. as johnson said some time ago — for a behavior to be different it must make a difference. social conventions, cognitive knowledge of the world, and linguistic rulegoverned behavior is competence. considerations for intervention: behavior goals i communicative acts (i.e. speakers intentions and effects on listeners requests, responses, statements). social roles (formal/informal, egalitarian/unequal, work/social). discourse rules (obligations and options of speakers and listeners, i.e. turn-taking, topic maintenance and shift, cohesion strategies). compensatory strategies: permit and encourage the use of alternative methods to carry out the goals and obligations of appropriate, effective, and successful discourse. some strategies. already identified are: repetition, grammatical and phonological simplifica : tion, reliance on nonverbal behavior to revise | utterances. one can easily see the important changes which have taken place with our shift in paradigms. one last contribution has to do with criteria for dismissal from remediation. prior to our shift we most often enrolled and dismissed clients with reference to their scores on standardized tests. in america, many guidelines set up by various states utilized a 1 or 2 standard deviation from the mean on a standardized test in the areas of phonology, syntax, and/or semantics. today, we are considering dismissal when we can document that a client has improved some aspect of the communicative system and is thus better able to manage relationships with others, through communication, with more ease and comfort. it is quite evident that theoretical changes have filtered down to the clinical treatment of clients. there is no doubt that the focus in the mid-eighties is on the social dimensions of communication. as johnson (1946) stated decades ago: "leaving any consideration of language behavior the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the pragmatics of language 5 out of a discussion of personality would be somewhat like leaving the cheese out of a cheese souffle. as a matter of fact, most of the key terms that we customarily use in talking about personality are seen, on close scrutiny, to refer somehow to reactions that are made to and with words and other symbols" (p. 243). why deal with the social aspects of communication alongside of linguistic and cognitive aspects? argyle (1983) has some startling evidence to support the fact that with a rich network of social relationships one lives longer and is happier. can we then think of any better reason for dealing with communicative behaviors from a social perspective? the communicative system is an individual's most powerful tool for getting along in this world. in steiner's, the portage to san cristobal of a. h. (1981) he describes words as able to heal, bless, cripple, and kill. there seems to be nothing which cannot be done with the power of words. for example one can see how hitler with his verbal and paralinguistic communicative abilities committed hideous crimes against all humanity. gandhi, on the other hand, communicated through silence and enriched the lives of millions. we are in an exciting and challenging era and those of us who call ourselves speech, language clinicians are in the business of social change. i do not imagine our clients will turn out to be any more homogeneous in the area of pragmatics than they are in the areas of phonology, syntax, and semantics. the shift is exhilarating. as is often the case this paradigm shift appears to be a revisit from the earlier days when our field was just developing — "we shall not cease from exploration and in the end of our exploring will be to arrive when we started and know the place for the first time." t.s. eliot references argyle, μ .visiting scholar, university of cape town, cape town, south africa, august 1983. austin, j. how to do things with words. cambridge: harvard university press, 1962. bates, e., benigni, l., bretherton, i., camaioni, l., and volterra, v. the emergence of symbols. new york: academic press, 1979. chomsky, n. syntactic structures. the hague: mouton, 1957. chomsky, n. aspects of a theory of syntax. cambridge: mit press, 1965. fuller, r.b. synergetics. new york: macmillan, 1975. gould, s. the mismeasure of man. new york: w.w. norton and co., 1981. humboldt, w. von. uberdie verschiedenheit des menschlichen sprachbaues. berlin, 1836. james. w. what pragmatism means pragmatism; a new name for some old ways of thinking. new york: longmans, green, & co., 1907. johnson, w. people in quandaries. new york: harper and brothers, 1946. kant, i. critique of pure reason. (first edition, 1781). new york: e.p. dutton & co., inc., 1978. kuhn, t. the structure of scientific revolution: chicago: university of chicago press, 1962. lewis, t. the youngest science. new york: the viking press, 1983. medawar, pb. advice to a young scientist. new york: harper and row, publishers, 1979. peirce, w. how to make ideas clear. popular science monthly, 1878, 12, 286-302. prutting, c.a. pragmatics as social competence. j. speech hear. dis., 1982, 47, 123-134. searle, j. speech acts: an essay in the philosophy of language. cambridge: university press, 1969. steiner, g. the portage to san cristobal ofa.h. new york: simon and schuster, 1981. wittgenstein, l. philosophical investigations. new york: macmillan publishing co., 1958. die suid-afrikaan.se tydskrif virkommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 55 word-finding strategies in closed head injured adults juleen kleiman, ba (speech and hearing therapy) (witwatersrand) lesley bucke, ba (speech and hearing therapy) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract word-finding difficulties are a common and prominent language deficit following closed head injury. the word-finding difficulties of three closed head injured patients were investigated within the framework of compensatory strategies using teicher's taxonomy of wordfinding strategies (teicher, 1986). the word-finding difficulties were evaluated during procedural discourse and two confrontation naming conditions. each subject's communicative competence and language ability was determined. results indicated that all subjects employed a wide range of strategies, particularly during confrontation naming, but with differential effectiveness. a relationship was noted between the strategy's effectiveness and the subject's pragmatic ability. the results are discussed in the light of the existing literature on head injury. the theoretical and clinical implications are considered. opsomming woordvindprobleme is 'n algemene en prominente taalverskynsel na geslote hoofbeserings. woordvindprobleme van proefpersone met geslote hoofbeserings is geevalueer binne 'n raamwerk van kompensasiestrategiee deur gebruik te maak van teicher se "taxonomy of word-finding strategies" (teicher, 1986). die woordvindprobleme is geevalueer tydens proseduregesprekke en twee konfrontasiebenoemingstake. elke proefpersoon se kommunikasieen taalvaardigheid is bepaal. die resultate dui daarop dat al die proefpersone gebruik gemaak het van 'n wye reeks stategiee, veral tydens konfrontasiebenoeming, maar met variasie in effektiwiteit. 'n ooreenkoms is opgemerk tussen die strategies wat gebruik is en die proefpersone se pragmatiese vermoens. die resultate is bespreek in die lig van reeds bestaande literatuur in verband met geslote hoofbeserings en daar is verwys na teoretiese en kliniese implikasies. closed head injury (chi) has been termed "the invisible epidemic" (holland, 1982) in view of the marked increase in the incidence of the disorder during this decade (annegers, grabouw, kurland and laws, 1980). the primary source of pathophysiology in chi is that of blunt trauma to the skull which results in multiple diffuse damage to the brain, discontinuation of neural substance and shearing and straining of the axons in j the white brain matter occurs (hagen, 1984). this results 'in a diversity of cognitive, memory and language deficits, based on the combination of pathophysiologic mechanisms and severity of the injury (ylivisaker and szekeres, 1986). the chi patient therefore presents the speech-language pathologist with a "unique and complex diagnostic, prognostic and treatment challenge" (hagen, 1984:245). the neurobehavioural and linguistic sequelae following chi constitutes an area of much controversy. different labels including "aphasia" (luria, 1970), "subclinical aphasia" (sarno, 1980) and "confused" language (hagen, 1984) have been adopted to describe the linguistic symptoms following chi. the heterogeneity of this population is being increasingly well recognised. despite this heterogeneity, researchers are in agreement that anomia is a common and prominent language deficit following chi (hagen, 1984; heilman, safron and geschwind, 1971; levin, benton and grossman, 1981). anomia has been defined as poor access to lexical items (word retrieval) and as a failure to name on confrontation and during discourse (goodglass, kaplan, weintraub and ackerman, 1976). the primary cortical areas identified as being responsible for naming abilities include the parietotemporo-occipital areas of the left dominant hemisphere (luria, 1975; benson, 1979). the chi patient frequently presents with circumlocutions, paraphasias and reduced fluency which are typical of aphasia (holland, 1982). in addition, chi patients reportedly exhibit "non-aphasic" naming errors resulting from their impulsivity, perceptual disinhibition and lack of language boundness (holland, 1982; teicher, 1986). within the past few years there has been a renewed focus on the importance of observing and assessing language and behaviour of the chi within a compensatory strategy framework? (kirchner and skarakis-doyle, 1983; penn, 1985; penn and cleary, 1987; teicher, 1986). when compensation follows a head injury we are observing the patient's direct attempt to compensate for his primary deficits. this is in accord with piaget's (1952) belief that the human organism constantly strives to maintain a balance and adapt to environmental demands. this process of equilibrium is determined in part by neurological and subject variables, and in part by ecologic variables (penn and cleary, 1987). compensatory strategies, which may develop spontaneously or which may be trained, have been defined as alternate methods of controlling and manipulating information (kirchner and skarakis-doyle, 1983). the individual adopts these strategies to meet the communicative demands of the situation when he is unable to bring together all the necessary resources, as a result of brain damage (penn, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 © sasha 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 j u l e e n kleiman and lesley bucke 1985). the degree to which a brain damaged patient will develop and utilise compensatory strategies will play a major role in determining the eventual outcome for the patient (penn, 1985). teicher (1986) examined the word-finding strategies employed by a group of chi individuals during an instance of word-finding difficulty. the results revealed that by utilising a behavioural taxonomy to assess word-finding strategies, many of the strategies employed by the subjects could be identified, described and compared within and across subjects. profiles of successful and unsuccessful strategies and their pragmatic effectiveness were identified. furthermore, the importance of evaluating naming ability during discourse, as well as confrontation naming was stressed. this study highlighted the potential value of the chi population's word-finding difficulties within a compensatory framework. according to ylvisaker and szekeres (1986:474) "impaired social interaction is often the residual deficit most troubling for the patient's family and for the patient himself." assessment tools which therefore have as their basis a social interaction model of communication which incorporates linguistic, pragmatic, social and cognitive variables are invaluable (penn and cleary, 1987). the purpose of this study therefore is to examine the wordfinding compensatory strategies being employed by three chi subjects during discourse and confrontation naming and to determine the effectiveness of these strategies in communicative interaction. by evaluating chi wordfinding difficulties in terms of compensatory strategies a pattern of behaviour may emerge which may offer insight into the central nervous sytem (marshall, 1976) as well as provide us with useful prognostic and diagnostic information. this will then assist us in designing more effective therapeutic strategies as the ultimate therapeutic goal, according to penn (1985), is to teach positive strategies which will increase pragmatic competence and flexibility of style. method aims the aim of the present study was to identify, describe and compare the word-finding strategies employed by three chi subjects during procedural discourse and confrontation naming tasks in an effort to adapt to their word-finding difficulties. the study was further designed to determine whether a relationship exists between the nature and the effectiveness of such strategies, and the individual's overall communicative effectiveness and language ability. subjects (ss) three english speaking subjects who had sustained closed head injuries as a result of motor vehicle accidents (ss 1 and 2) and assault (s3) were selected as subjects for this study. ss 1 and 3 were males, s2 female and their age range was 27.1 years to 44.4 years (mean 33.3 years). mean coma duration of the subjects was 4 weeks (range 2 — 6 weeks), thus indicating a severe head injury according to the criteria suggested by russell (1971). all subjects were neurologically stable at the time of testing with a mean post-injury time of 21 months (range 8 — 48 months). subjects were all pretraumatically right-handed and had completed at least 11 years of schooling. family reports indicated that subjects were of average intellectual capacity prior to the accident. no previous history of sensory or motor deficit was reported nor was any history of drug or alcohol abuse noted. hearing was within normal limits for all three subjects at the time of testing. further relevant clinical and biographical details are included in table 1 below. testing material as there are presently no language tests designed specifically for the chi population it was necessary to employ and table 1: relevant clinical and biographical detail for all subjects subject 1 subject 2 subject 3 age 27.1 years 28.3 years 44.4 years , sex male female male j educational standard 9 apprentice matric diploma greenoaks secretarial college standard 8 j • 1 premorbid laterality right right right <' | coma (weeks) 3 6 2 ! time since injury to testing (months) 48 8 8 history of speech therapy yes, until 1984 presently receiving theraphy yes, until 1987 present motor problems l-arm hemiplegia l-hemiplegia. previous l-side neglect resolved l-hemiplegia occupation supervising packing of medical supplies unemployed unemployed ** / immediate post-trauma cat scan results diffuse cerebral oedema with small parieto-temporal haemorrhage on left diffuse cerebral oedema with parieto-occipital haemorrhage on right intracerebral haemorrhage occupying most of right temporal lobe with right and left lateral ventricle involvement. craniotomy to drain sub-dural haemotoma the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) word-finding strategies in closed head injured adults 57 modify tests designed for other neurogenic groups such as aphasia. the following tests were administered: a. western aphasia battery (wab) (kertesz, 1980) the aphasia quotient (aq) was only administered as it serves as a functional measure of spoken language impairment. it thus provided an overall language severity score in subjects and a basis for comparison between subjects. b. prutting's pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982) — a strong relationship seems to exist between the chi ss' effective use of compensatory strategies and their overall communicative competence (cleary, 1985; teicher, 1986). the general pragmatic communication abilities of the ss were therefore assessed using prutting's pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982). this provides a global measure of communicative competence and places language in the context of socialisation. the data for the analysis consisted of a 10 minute interactional language sample which was simultaneously videoand audiotaped. topics discussed in this sequence were: family structure, the subject's accident, daily activities and interests. c. discourse tasks — this is a procedure for sampling and quantifying connective speech for chi ss who display social and vocational communicative deficits yet have difficulty on standardised aphasia tests (milton, prutting and binder, 1984). procedural discourse was employed as the mode of elicitation and it provides the tester with a moderate level of context control and has temporal constraints (ulatowska, north and macaluso-haynes, 1981). the task involved a complete discussion of three daily activities noted for their familiarity and relatively high frequency of occurrence for an adult (ulatowska et al. 1981). instructions given were: "i'd like you to tell me as completely as possible, how to make a sandwich; change / a bed; wash laundry." d. confrontation naming tests — chosen to evaluate different aspects of confrontation naming. the boston naming test (bnt) (kaplan, goodglass and weintraub, 1976) was employed to evaluate object (noun) naming on confrontation, while the action naming test (ant) (obler and albert, 1986) was employed to evaluate action (verb) naming on confrontation. repetition and cueing were provided if the subject did not respond after a 20 second delay. behaviours associated with word-finding difficulties on the procedural discourse tasks, the bnt and the ant, were described using teicher's behavioural taxonomy of word-finding strategies (teicher, 1986), based on penn's taxonomy of compensatory strategies (1985). this qualitative analysis yields pertinent information about language use difficulties in chi. five broad strategy categories were derived and are displayed in table 2. an additional category for analysis "repetition" was included in the taxonomy as penn (1985) makes use of this strategy in her taxonomy of compensatory strategies. repetition facilitates processing and captures the listener's attention. a 'detailed description and example of each word-finding strategy is displayed in appendix i. table 2: teicher's taxonomy of word-finding strategies (teicher, 1985) elaboration fluency description circumlocution silent pause filled pause non-verbal/paralinguistic social symbolic noise meaningless gesture iconic/pantomime facial expression deictic comment clause self-correction requests help other any behaviour not included in the preceding categories testing procedure testing was carried out individually and extended over a number of morning sessions in an attempt to eliminate fatigue as a confounding variable. in order to reduce anxiety, rapport was established with each subject prior to his being tested. analysis of data standardised tests administered were scored according to the procedure laid down in the test manual. non-standardised test results were analysed according to the methods described previously. the examiner and two reliability judges completed prutting's pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982) independently in order to measure reliability. point-to-point inter-rater reliability was calculated separately for the three categories of the protocol. reliability was 82%, with a range extending from 75,5% to 85,5%. rating was thus reliable, based on silverman's (1977:157) minimal acceptable inter-rater reliability of 0,75. three raters rated each ss' word-finding strategies using teicher's taxonomy of word-finding strategies (teicher, 1986). the raters involved one of the authors and two third year speech therapy students blind to the aims of the study. all raters had been trained in the use of the profile. training sessions included familiarisation, discussion and explanation of each strategy listed on the taxonomy. clinical videotapes of chi ss engaged in conversation were chosen to illustrate and clarify certain strategies. judges rated each interaction separately and independently on all three viewings and completed the ratings according to their observation of relevant dimensions. during the first viewing, raters were required to underline when a word-finding difficulty had occurred. during the second viewing, the raters were required to comment if the word search was successful or unsuccessful. a successful word search was any attempt at word-finding which resulted in the production of a specific lexical item. in an unsuccessful word search, the listener may not have known the desired word, but knew when the speaker had not reached the target word or when the search strategy was ineffective die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 j u l e e n kleiman and lesley bucke (teicher, 1986). finally, the raters were required to categorise the behaviours accompanying the word-finding instances into teicher's behavioural taxonomy (teicher, 1986). point to point inter-rater reliability was determined. reliability values ranged from 83.2% for s2 to 77.4% for si. it can therefore be concluded that rating was reliable according to silverman's (1977; 157) minimal acceptable interrater reliability value of 0.75. duration of each word search for all ss was analysed quantitatively by' computing the overall ratings of successful and unsuccessful searches. it was calculated from the moment of onset of the search to the moment of successful or unsuccessful achievement and then rounded to the nearest half second. finally, the percentage of time each initial word-finding strategy and strategy sequence was employed successfully and unsuccessfully was computed for each subject. results and discussion a. western aphasia battery (kertesz, 1980) subject 1 obtained a wab aphasia quotient of 74.8 while ss 2 and 3 scored 92.6 and 64.0 respectively. none of the ss therefore scored above 93.8 which is the suggested cut-off point for aphasia (kertesz, 1980). this indicates an element of language form disturbance for all ss, the nature of which was diffuse. this contradicts milton, prutting and binder's (1984) opinion that chi ss do not present with a degree of aphasia. word-finding difficulties were observed in all ss in addition to problems in the areas of comprehension and repetition which correlates with cleary's (1985) findings. brooks (1984), cleary (1985) and groher (1977) have suggested that an underlying memory problem together with reduced attention and concentration span are contributory factors towards overall difficulties in these areas. naming ability and verbal associative fluency have been shown to be the most effective predictors of injury (levin et al. 1981) which corresponds with the findings. s3 presented with the most severe generalised cerebral dysfunction in addition to the most severe linguistic deficit. b. prutting's pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982) all 3 ss presented with a greater or lesser degree of pragmatic difficulty. s2 experienced the least overall difficulty (15% inappropriate responses), si presented with 22% inappropriate responses while s3 experienced the most overall difficulty (66%). similar findings were found on the wab (kertesz, 1980). this observation contributes towards irvine and behrmann's (1986) finding of a balance existing between language form and language use skills in chi individuals. results indicate that overall appropriateness of subjects' responses occurred mainly in the utterance act and the propositional act categories while the least appropriate response occurred in the ilocutionary/perlocutionary act. this is in agreement with cleary (1985), milton et al. (1984), and teicher (1986) and it seems to imply that chi individuals function inadequately in their roles as discourse partners. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 table 3: subjects' performance on the procedural discourse tasks si s2 s3 number of word-finding difficulties 32 7 19 percentage: successful 69% 86% 74% percentage: unsuccessful 31% 14% 26% , duration: successful mean range 2.0 sec 0.5 — 5.0 sec 1.0 sec 0.5 — 1.0 sec 2.0 sec | 1.0 — 5.0 sec ' duration: unsuccessful mean range 3.5 sec 0.5 — 7.0 sec 2.0 sec 4.0 sec ; 2.0 5.0 sec j number of different strategies used: ' successful 7 2 ! number of different strategies used: ' unsuccessful 7 2 4 number of different strategy sequences: successful 5 0 3 number of different strategy sequences: unsuccessful 6 1 2 most frequently used initial strategy: successful filled pause 37% silent delay 86% filled pause 58% most frequently used initial strategy: unsuccessful filled pause 30% repetition 30% silent delay 100% filled pause 100% most frequently used stategy sequence: successful filled pause/ repetition 23% / filled pause/ silent delay ' 2 1 % most frequently used stategy sequence: unsuccessful filled pause/ repetition 20% filled pause/ description 60% r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) word-finding strategies in closed head injured adults 59 c. procedural discourse tasks while all 3 subjects presented with word-finding difficulties they all experienced relatively high levels of success for production of the desired lexical item following this difficulty. table 3 describes the subjects' performance during the procedural discourse tasks. both the duration mean and range of word searches were calculated. all ss presented with a longer duration when strategies were unsuccessful. for example s3's mean duration of a successful word search was 2 seconds while that of an unsuccessful word search was 4 seconds, a difference of 2 seconds. a wide range of successful and unsuccessful initial word-finding strategies were employed, namely filled pause, silent delay and repetition, though a greater number and variation of strategies were used when success was not attained. this may relate to the ss' attempts to compensate for increased instances of word-finding difficulty. certain strategies occurred more frequently than others, particularly fluency strategies. silent delay and filled pause were employed 69% of the time during a successful word search and 75% during an unsuccessful word search. repetition was the most facilitative initial strategy and was employed by ss to maintain listener attention (penn, 1985). ss 1 and 3 employed word-finding strategy sequences, successfully and unsuccessfully, in an attempt to produce desired lexical items. filled pause was the most frequently used initial strategy in all sequences. filled pause may be potentially distracting if used too often (cleary, 1985). this was particularly evident in si's speech where it was the most frequently table 4: subjects' performance on the bnt used initial strategy and part of the most frequently used strategy sequence, both successfully and unsuccessfully. this may relate to the increased number of word-finding difficulties experienced by si. d. confrontation naming tests — boston naming test (bnt) (kaplan et al. 1976) all ss experienced word-finding difficulties and, as a result, employed word-finding strategies to overcome this difficulty. table 4 illustrates the ss' performance on the bnt (kaplan et al. 1976). the level of success for production of the desired lexical item was relatively low for all ss — 16%, 20% and 42% for ss 1, 2 and 3 respectively. the duration mean and range of word searches indicates that all ss presented with a longer duration when strategies were unsuccessful, for example 8 seconds versus 2 seconds for s3. the ss' results differed from each other for successful and unsuccessful initial word-finding strategies. however, for all ss, an increase in the number and variation of different initial strategies and word-finding strategy sequences was evident when success was unattainable. all ss employed word-finding strategy sequences, but the most frequently used successful and unsuccessful strategy sequences varied across and within ss. s2, however, employed the same successful and unsuccessful strategy sequences, silent delay/description. this is hypothesised to relate to s2's careful self-monitoring. elaboration strategies (description and elimination), fluency si s2 s3 number of word-finding difficulties 49 30 19 percentage: successful 16% 20% 42% percentage: unsuccessful 84% 80% 58% / / / percentage: successful with cueing phonemic cues 34% 56% 57% / / / percentage: stimulus cues 0% 44% 0% duration: successful mean range 6.0 sec 1.0 20.5 sec 6.5 sec 2.0 10.0 sec 2.0 sec 0.5 3.5 sec duration: unsuccessful mean range 10.0 sec 2.5 19.0 sec 8.5 sec 2.5 14.5 sec 8.0 sec 2.5 11.5 sec . / , successful 5 3 5 t inumder o r u m e r e n t s t r a t e g i e s u s e u . unsuccessful 11 6 5 successful 4 2 1 inumder o i u m e r e n t s t r a t e g y s e q u e n c e s . unsuccessful 14 11 5 most frequently used initial strategy: successful repetition 50% silent delay 50% repetition 63% most frequently used initial strategy: unsuccessful description 50% description 45% description 73% successful repetition/ description 25% silent delay/ description 33% comment/ filled pause 13% iviost irequentiy useu stategy sequence: 1 unsuccessful description/ comment clause 13% silent delay/ description 30% description/ filled pause 28% die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieawykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 j u l e e n kleiman and lesley bucke strategies (silent delay and filled pause) and social strategies (comment clause, self-correction and requests help) were the most frequently used initial strategies. all the ss required cues following the failure to name the desired lexical item. phonemic cues were the most successful in facilitating retrieval of the specific lexical item for all ss. — action naming tests (ant) (obler and albert, 1986) all 3 ss experienced instances of word-finding difficulty on the ant. they employed word-finding strategies to overcome their difficulty. table 5 illustrates the ss' performance on the ant (obler and albert, 1986). level of success for production of the desired lexical item was low for all ss — 27%, 22% and 30% for ss 1, 2 and 3 respectively. results obtained correlate closely with those obtained on the β nt (kaplan et al. 1976). ss presented with longer duration when strategies were unsuccessful in facilitating production of the specific lexical item. for all ss an increase in the number of different initial strategies and strategy sequences was evident when success was not attained. s3, however, deviated from the trend. the number of different initial strategies employed during the bnt (kaplan et al. 1976) and the ant (obler and albert, 1986) for successful and unsuccessful word searches was equivalent. si and s2 employed word-finding strategy sequences while s3 failed to successfully employ strategy sequences. this is hypothesised to relate to s3's inappropriate pragmatic behaviour and lack of self-monitoring. the most frequently used successful and unsuccessful strategy sequences varied across and within ss except s2 who employed the same strategy sequence (silent delay — description) successfully and unsuccessfully.' this was also evident on the bnt (kaplan et al. 1976). fluency and elaboration strategies were the most frequently employed initial word-finding strategies. all ss required cues following the failure to name the desired word, and phonemic cues were the most successful in facilitating retrieval of the specific lexical item. the superiority of phonemic cues over stimulus and verbal contextual cues is confirmed by the results on both confrontation naming tests and by the studies performed by pease and goodglass (1978) and williams (1983). e. summary of results and discussion the results indicate that three different patterns of performance were identified with some commonality of wordfinding strategies employed across ss. the study indicates that a relationship exists between ss' effective use of wordfinding strategies during procedural discourse tasks and their overall communicative competence, as shown on prutting's pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982) and the wab. viewed within a compensatory strategy framework, it is advocated that the chi ss are self-monitoring more carefully in an attempt to compensate for their deficits; hence the use of word-finding strategies. this is in accord with cleary table 5: subjects' performance on the ant si s2 s3 number of word-finding difficulties 22 18 17 percentage: successful 27% 22% 30% percentage: unsuccessful 73% 78% 70% successful with percentage: cueing phonemic cues 61% 83% 53% percentage: verbal contextual cues 45% 56% 29% successful mean 5.0 sec 9.5 sec 3.0 sec duration: range 2.0 8.0 sec 4.5 13.0 sec 1.5 5.0 sec duration: unsuccessful mean 11.0 sec 10.0 sec 6.5 sec range 5.5 18.5 sec 4.5 18.0 sec 0.5 16.5 sec number of different strategies used: successful 4 3 3 number of different strategies used: unsuccessful 7 4 3 number of different strategy sequences: successful 3 2 0 number of different strategy sequences: unsuccessful 7 8 1 successful filled pause 38% description 50% self-correct 40% filled pause most frequently used initial strategy: 40% most frequently used initial strategy: unsuccessful silent delay 38% description 57% description 50% most frequently used stategy sequence: successful repetition/ pantomime 33% silent delay/ description 40% most frequently used stategy sequence: unsuccessful filled pause/ description 32% silent delay/ description 35% description/ filled pause 16% the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) word-finding strategies in closed head injured adults 61 (1985) and teicher (1986). in terms of word-finding difficulty, a confrontation naming task was found to be more difficult for chi ss than a procedural discourse task. while procedural discourse maximally challenges the chi individual's communicative ability, it may facilitate naming as found in non-brain damaged individuals (clark and clark, 1977). the semantic, syntactic and temporal constraints characteristic of procedural discourse (ulatowska et al. 1981) seemed to assist ss 2 and 3 in the recall and production of the desired lexical items as the "lost" word seemed to be closer to the "tip of the tongue". within the confrontation naming task, naming of objects (nouns) appeared to be more difficult than the naming of actions (verbs). this finding is consistent with the findings of goodglass et al.'s (1966) study and irvine and behrmann's (1986) study. mean length of response time for successful and unsuccessful searches during the procedural discourse tasks was less than during the confrontation naming condition for all 3 ss. this is hypothesised to relate to the nature of the discourse task which may have facilitated naming. this finding is confirmed by newcombe et al. (1965, cited by berko-gleason, goodglass, obler, greene, hyde and weintraub, 1980) and teicher (1986). si presented with a longer successful mean latency for object naming versus action naming whereas the converse was found for ss2 and 3. furthermore individual patterns exist for unsuccessful mean latencies for object and action naming. it is evident from the data that no one pattern of performance is generated, confirming the notion of heterogeneity within the chi population (hagen, 1984; irvine and behrmann, 1986). in contrast to this diverse picture, all ss presented with a longer duration for unsuccessful word-finding strategies during procedural discourse and confrontation naming. pausing to reconsider and reattempt when not experiencing success is speculated to contribute to this longer duration as chi ss experience significant difficulty processing complex information (hagen, 1984). i i alkss displayed an increase in the number of different initial strategies and different strategy sequences when success was not attained. it appears that in an attempt to overcome the unsuccessful attempt, additional and varied strategies are employed. this appears to confirm a breakdown in luria's (1975) 'rule of forcej theory where strong and weak stimuli evoke similar responses so that no selective organisation of the relevant associational process is possible. an analysis of the entire sequence of strategies employed reveals much variability across and within ss. the same initial strategies were used successfully and unsuccessfully by all the ss during the procedural discourse tasks and occasionally during the confrontation naming tasks, namely fluency strategies. this finding correlates with cleary's (1985) and teicher's (1986) results. ulatowska et al. (1981) state that in discourse, message transmission in any form is the goal. therefore the chi ss, in an attempt to maintain the conversational flow and transmit the message, predominantly adopted fluency strategies. other strategies employed which were not accounted for by teicher's behavioural taxonomy of word-finding strategies (1986) were the use of unfinished words, okay', and humour. these strategies have implications in the development of assessment tools for chi and in their treatment. in summary, word-finding strategies were employed by all the subjects in an attempt to overcome their word-finding difficulty, but to differential effect. there are several possible explanations for the performance variation noted. coma duration and neurological dynamics may account for the differences. other variables which may play a role are age range, the time-since-injury and non-medical variables such as personality and motivation since affective disorders are a well-recognised by-product of chi (hagen, 1984). in addition, the increasingly well accepted fact that the chi group is a heterogeneous group is a further important consideration. individual patterns, rather than a single group pattern, characterise this population (irvine and behrmann, 1986), a finding supported by this data. conclusion it is clear from this study that chi ss are generally using word-finding strategies frequently and effectively to overcome their word-finding difficulties. this is in agreement with the results of teicher's (1986) study. this may indicate that chi ss are, in fact, effective communicators who learn useful compensatory strategies if given the opportunity to do so (cleary, 1985; teicher, 1986). the implications of this finding for remediation are many. in view of the heterogeneity of the chi population, it is essential that the clinician assesses each closed head injured patient individually without relying on the predicted findings from the literature. the need for sensitive assessment measures aimed at evaluating communicative efficiency in chi is stressed. measures such as teicher's taxonomy (1986) may be employed to present a profile of compensatory strategies. the speech-language pathologist may then structure an effective treatment programme as "... treatment serves to challenge and channel spontaneous recovery, maximise residual function, and compensate for lost abilities." (hagen, 1984:275.) references annegers, j.f., grabouw, j.d., kurland, l.t. and laws, e.r. the incidence, causes and secural trends of head trauma in olmsted country, minnesota. neurology, 30, 912—919, 1980. benson, d.f. neurological correlates of anomia. in h. whitaker and h. whitaker (eds.) studies in neurolinguistics. new york: academic press, 1979. berko-gleason, k., goodglass, h., obler, l., greene, e., hyde, m.r., and weintraub, s. narrative strategies of aphasic and normal speaking subjects. journal of speech and hearing research, 23, 3 7 0 3 8 2 , 1980. brooks, n. cognitive deficits after head injury. in n. brooks (ed.) closed head injury — psychological, social and family consequences. new york: oxford university press, 1984. clark, h. and clark, e. psychology and language: an introduction to psycholinguistics. new york: harcourt brace jovanovich, 1977. cleary, j. compensatory strategies in closed head injury. undergraduate research report, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, 1985. goodglass, h., kaplan, e., weintraub, s. and ackerman, n. the "tip-of-the-tongue" phenomenon in aphasia. cortex, 12, 1 4 5 1 5 3 , 1976. groher, m. language and memory disorders following closed head trauma. journal of speech and hearing research, 20, 2 1 2 2 2 3 , 1977. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 juleen kleiman and lesley bucke hagen, c. language disorders in head trauma. in a. holland (ed.) language disorders in adults. san diego: college-hill press, 1984. heilman, k., safron, a. and geschwind, n. closed head trauma and aphasia. journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, 34, 2 6 5 2 6 9 , 1971. holland, a. when is aphasia aphasia? the problem of closed head injury. paper presented at the clinical aphasiology conference, oshkosh, wisconsin, 1982. irvine, l. and behrmann, m. the communicative and cognitive deficits following closed head injury. south african journal of communication disorders, 33, 49—54, 1986. kaplan, e., goodglass, h. and weintraub, s. the boston naming test. unpublished test, boston, m.a. : veterans hospital, 1976. kertesz, a. western aphasia battery. london, ontario, canada: university of western ontario, 1980. kirchner, d. and skarakis-doyle, e. developmental language disorders: a theoretical perspective. in t. gallagher and c. prutting (eds.) pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. san diego: college-hill press, 1983 levin, h.s., benton, a.l. and grossman, r.g. neurobehavioural consequences of closed head injury. new york: oxford university press, 1981. luria, a.r. traumatic aphasia: its syndromes, psychology and treatment. the hague: mouton, 1970. luria, a.r. basic problems of language in the light of psychology and neurolinguistics. in e. lennenberg and e. lennenberg (eds.) foundations of language development. new york: academic press, 1975. marshall, r.c. word retrieval of aphasic adults. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 61, 444—451, 1976. milton, s.b., prutting, c.a. and binder, g.m. appraisal of communicative competence in head injured adults. paper presented at the clinical aphasiology conference, seabrook island, california, 1984. obler, l.k. and albert, m.l. the action naming test, 1986. pease, d. and goodglass, h. the effects of cueing on picture naming in aphasia. cortex, 14, 178—189, 1978. penn, c. compensatory strategies in aphasia: behavioural and neurological correlates. in k.m. grieve and d. griesel (eds.) neuropsychology. university of south africa press, muckleneuk, pretoria, 1985. penn, c. and cleary, j. compensatory strategies in the language of closed head injured patients. unpublished paper, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, 1987. piaget, j. the origin of intelligence in children. new york: basic books, 1952. prutting, c.a. pragmatics as social competence. journal of speech and hearing disorders. 47, 123—134, 1982. russell, w.r. the traumatic amnesias. london: oxford university press, 1971. sarno, m.t. the nature of verbal impairment after closed head injury. journal of nervous and mental disorders, 1968, 6 8 5 6 9 2 , 1980. silverman, s. research design in speech pathology and audiology, engelwood cliffs, new jersey : prentice hall inc., 1977. teicher, s.h. word-finding strategies in closed head injured adults. masters dissertation, university of california, santa barbara, 1986. ulatowska, h., north, α., macaluso-haynes, s. production of narrative and procedural discourse in aphasia. brain and langauge, 13, 3 4 5 3 7 1 , 1981. williams, s. factors influencing naming performance in aphasia: a review of the literature. journal of communication disorders, 16, 357—372, 1983. ylivisaker, m. and szekeres, s.f. management of the patient with closed head injury. in r. chapey (ed.) language intervention strategy in adult aphasia (2nd ed). williams and wilkins, baltimore: 1986. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) word-finding strategies in closed head injured adults 6 3 appendix 1 description of teicher's (1986) taxonomy of word-finding strategies strategy type definition examples elaboration — description — circumlocution — elimination fluency — silent delay — filled pause social — comment clause — self-correction — requests help non-verbal/ paralinguistic — symbolic noise meaningless gesture iconic gesture/ pantomime — facial expression — deictic other — incorrect lexical choice expansion or elaboration of the message to ensure effective transmission. description of the appearance or function of what is being spoken about. production of general words or "empty words" (heilman et al. 1971) in place of the desired word. elimination of a series of words which may be semantically related to the desired word. used to hold the place in conversation. an unfilled pause. pause filled with some sound indicating ongoing lexical search. devices reflecting sensitivity to the needs of the interlocuter. any metalanguage about the desired word search used to maintain a speaking turn. correction of phonetic or semantic errors without cues from the listener. direct request or a non-verbal cue for help. vocal and/or gestural behaviours which supplement or substitute for the verbal message. ί sound effects made in conjunction with or in place of the intended word to ensure effective transmission. meaningless, natural gesture, lacking structure. clearly, discernible gesture representing a particular word, object or its use. facial configuration in conjunction with or in place of the intended lexical item. pointing to an object or in a general direction. any behaviour unclassifiable in the preceding taxonomy synonym or any acceptable alternative for the desired lexical item. ' ί have one at home — it's white and you dry yourself with it." (towel) "you get a free whatschimacallit." "it's not january, it's february." "we use the uh, uhm the book." "you know" or "oh, what are those things?" "he hit the bat, i mean the ball." "i'll know if you give me the first sound", or use of gesture. the use of a high pitched, glottalised screech made to represent a car's brakes. an upward and outward hand movement. left hand in a cupped position as if holding a jar and the right hand moving in a circular motion above the left as if opening a jar. widening of eyes and opening of mouth to indicate surprise. "over there" or "that one". "digging" for "pushing". die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) furnquip manufacturing and wholesale suppliers (pty) limited registration no. 61/02456/07 t / a furnqup medical group your yellow pages of medical equipment head office correspondence only to: p.o. box 84416 greenside 2034 south africa tel. a d d . furnquip tel: (011) 787-5733/4, 787-5768/9 head office: "greenbriars" 75 conrad drive, blairgowrie 2194, south africa telex 4-28072 sa. fax (2711) 787-5716 associated companies deliveries store only furnquip medical natal (pty) ltd furnquip medical transkei medical industries c / o dolphins transport p.o. box 1999, bophuthatswana (pty) ltd (pty) ltd joe arnison str., durban 4000 p.o. box 84416, p.o. box 9030 madeira str., labore tel. 031-309-2313/4 greenside 2034 umtata 5100 brakpan telex 6-22133 tel. (011) 787-5717 tel. 0471-24221 electronic artificial larynx a n d complete speech therapy aids fq/jm001 — barts artificial larynx mark ii this has been improved and although an old design, is still very useful as a back-up larynx or as a teaching aid. fq/jm005, 006, 007, 008 these are the different microphones that are available for use with the de luxe speech amplifier (010) and continue in production unchanged. fq/jm010 de luxe speech amplifier m k ii this has been completely redesigned. output has been increased by approximately one third and a new loudspeaker used which gives a much clearer and more crisp tone. fq/jm011 — m a r k ii electronic artificial larynx this unit has been improved and now has a separate on/off isolator switch to prevent accidental use in one's pocket etc. the output is increased and if the optional high power battery is used very great volume can be achieved. this unit is supplied with one battery and charger. fq/jm015 pausaid this is supplied complete with a microphone and battery. pausaid has a throat microphone which will detect when phonation is continuous and will therefore prompt the patient auditorially to make regular stops in speech. fq/jm016 — vocal loudness indicator the vli is a compact battery operated device intended to display relative levels of vocal loudness. the vli consists of a series of eight numbered lights, each lighting up at a different loudness level. fq/jm017 — companion intra oral larynx 1 introduced early 1987, this has proved extremely suej cessful as an intra-oral larynx for post operative use and for further information please fill in the following in some cases for permanent use by laryngectomies and return: where a throat larynx is not liked or acceptable. ! surname: fq/jm018 s u m m i t amplifier ' this is an amplifier in the same housing as the mark ii first names: 010 amplifier. address: fq/jm019 headset or throat microphone for the new type amplifier 018. fq/jm020 pocket audiometer ^ this unit is designed as a front line free field screening telephone no: audiometer capable of quickly and easily checking whether or not a patient has a hearing problem. v ^ s ^ f k directors: g.l.l. harding (group managing) j.l. m c f a r l a n d (rn. dip. nurs. sci) consultant national (british) m everything for hospitals and institutions under one roof ^f^s^ "the original turnkey project specialists" r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 15 videofluorografiese ondersoek van die slukproses by serebraal gestremde kinders videofluorografiese ondersoek van die slukproses by serebraal gestremde kinders karin le roux, β log (pretoria) anita van der merwe, d phil (pretoria) brenda louw, d phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria, pretoria opsomming die slukprobleme van serebraal gestremde kinders word klinies aan die hand van subjektiewe kriteria omskryf weinig navorsing is nog uitgevoer om die aard van hierdie probleme objektief en wetenskaplik te beskryf. die doel van hierdie studie was om die slukpatrone van twee ouderdomsgroepe serebraal gestremde kinders deur middel van videofluorografie te ondersoek. tien serebraal gestremde kinders tussen die ouderdomme van vier en dertienjaar het as proefpersonegedien. tydens die videofluorografiese evaluasie waar daar van die proefpersone vereis is om 'n vloeistof te sluk, is sekere aspekte van die oraal-voorbereidende, or ale en faringale fases van die slukproses geevalueer. die resultate van die studie toon aan dat slukprobleme wel voorkom by serebraal gestremde kinders. probleme kom hoofeaaklik voor tydens die oralefase en neem die vorm aan van byvoorbeeld onvoldoende tongpalatumkontak en swak beheer van die bolus. diejongergroep kinders vertoon meer slukprobleme as die ouer groep. by diejongergroep speel die tipeserebralegestremdheid skynbaargeen rol by die aard van die slukprobleme nie, terwyl daar by die ouer groep aanduidings is dat die tipe serebrale gestremdheid wel 'n rol speel. die resultate van die studie toon aan dat videofluorografie effektief geimplimenteer kan word om slukprobleme by serebraal gestremdes wetenskaplik te evalueer. abstract the swallowing problems of cerebral palsied children are clinically evaluated by means of subjective criteria. little research has been done to analyse these problems objectively and scientifically. the aim ofthis study was to analyse the swallowing patterns of cerebral palsied children in two age groups by means of videofluorography. ten cerebral palsied children between the ages of four and thirteen years were selected as subjects. during the investigation they were asked to swallow a liquid and certain aspects of the oral preparatory, oral and pharyngeal phases of swallowing were analysed. the results of the study indicated that swallowing problems do occur in cerebral palsied children, mainly during the oral phase, and in some cases take on the form ofinadequate contact between the tongue and palate and poor control of the bolus. the group ofyounger children had more swallowing problems than the older group. in theformer group the type of cerebral palsy did not seem to play any role in the swallowing problems which occurred while in the older group there were indications that the type of cerebral palsy could p6ssibly play a role. the results of the study indicate that videofluorography can be implemented effectively to scientifically evaluate the / swallowing problems of cerebral palsied children. voedingsprobleme asook swak speekselbeheer is van die grootste probleme wat die serebraal gestremde persoon ervaar. afwykings in die slukproses vorm deel van en dra by tot die voedingsprobleme van hierdie populasie. voedingsprobleme word hoofsaaklik as 'n geheel beskryf en min literatuur bestaan oor afwykings in die slukproses in besonder. uit 'n oorsig van die literatuur blyk dit dat die serebraal gestremde kind wel soms probleme ondervind met sluk (love, hagerman & tiami, 1980; hardy, 1983; logemann, 1983). die aard van die slukprobleme is nog grootliks onbekend en moontlike verskille ten opsigte van slukpatrone soos dit voorkom in die hoofgroepe serebraal gestremdes word nie in die literatuur omskryf nie. kliniese ondervindingtoon datevaluasiemetodes van die slukprobleme by hierdie kinders, oor die algemeen, op subjektiewe beoordeling tydens orofasiale ondersoeke berus (steenkamp, 1988). 'n leemte bestaan dus in die kliniese uitvoering van wetenskaplike, objektiewe en betroubare evaluering van slukprobleme by serebraal gestremdes (hardy, 1983). 'n videofluorografiese ondersoek bied die geleentheid om die totale slukproses objektief waar te neem en die spesifieke probleme in die oraal-voorbereidende, orale en faringale fases te identifiseer. die moontlike verband wat tussen voedingsprobleme en veral slukprobleme by babas kan bestaan, en die ontwikkeling van abnormale spraakpatrone in latere jare word tans algemeen aanvaar. identifisering en remediering van die abnormale bewegingspatrone in voeding en veral sluk van serebraal gestremde kinders, is dus noodsaaklik om 'n meer normale basis vir spraak daar te stel (mueller, 1976; morris, 1985). voedingsprobleme, onder andere slukprobleme en swak speekselbeheer is die gevolg van abnormale orale reflekspatrone en posturale afwykings van die hele liggaam. abnormale spiertonus, tongretraksie of stoot, retraksie van die lippe met gevolglike swak/geen lipsluiting, tongimmobiliteit, onstabiliteit van die mandibula, swak gradering van mandibulabewegings, verhoogde orale sensitiwiteit en kopen nekhiperekstensie bei'nvloed die slukproses onder andere en gee aanleiding tot voedingsprobleme (mueller, 1976; alexander, 1987; van der walt, 1988; steenkamp, 1989). die neuromotoriese probleme verhoed onder meer vorming en effektiewe hantering van die bolus tydens die slukproses. die gedisorganiseerde linguale bewegings verhinder weer gladde peristaltiese bewegings van die tong wat nodig is vir sluk. minder is bekend die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 karin le roux, anita van der merwe e n brenda louw oor slukafwykings in die faringale en esofagale fases. 'n ondersoek van 40 serebraal gestremde kinders by die northwestern university medical school het aangetoon dat 'n vertraagde slukrefleks en beperkte faringale peristalsis die probleme met die hoogste voorkoms in die faringale fase was (logeman 1983 : 216). die rol wat die lokalisasie van die neuromotoriese patologie speel by die aard van die slukprobleme is onbekend. die drie hoofgroepe van serebrale gestremdheid, naamlik spastiese, atetotiese en ataktiese tipes het die moontlikheid van differensiele aantasting van die slukproses. differensiele aantasting kan te wyte wees aan die verskillende vorms van neuromotoriese aantasting veroorsaak deur letsels in die verskillende dele van die neuromotoriese stelsel (hardy, 1983). slukprobleme word deur middel van verskeie evaluasiemetodes bestudeer (love et al. 1980; lear, flanagan & moorrees, 1965; logemann, 1983). videofluorografie verskaf egter die eerste maal die geleentheid om die slukproses akkuraat en objektief te kan bestudeer (logemann, 1983; brogan, foulner & turner, 1987). aangesien sluk 'n vinnige en dinamiese proses is, waar normale orale en faringale deurgangstye elk slegs 'n maksimum van 1 sekonde duur, sal die stadige raam-virraam opnames van videofluorografie, bydra tot 'n objektiewe beoordeling en herhaalde ondersoek van sluk (bzoch, 1979; logemann, 1983; brogan et al. 1987). inligting verkry met behulp van videofluorografie kan deur die terapeut gebruik word om neuromotoriese afwykings teenwoordig in die pasient se slukproses, aan te dui. sodanige inligting maak dit moontlik om te bepaal watter konsistensie voedsel geskik sal wees vir voeding, en identifiseer ook direkte sowel as indirekte behandelingsprosedures ten opsigte van die spesifieke probleem. mueller (1976:113) het die belangrike stelling gemaak dat 'n serebraal gestremde kind gehelp kan word slegs deur noukeurige waarneming en analise van sy vermoens en onvermoens. : dit blyk dus dat talle faktore kan bydra, of aanleiding gee, tot slukprobleme by serebraal gestremdes en dat die aard van die slukprobleme moontlik by die verskillende tipes serebrale gestremdheid kan verskil. objektiewe beoordeling van die slukproses met behulp van tegnieke soos videofluorografie kan inligting verskaf wat die aard van die probleme kan toelig. tot op hede is die voedingsprobleme van serebraal gestremdes egter slegs in die algemeen beskryf en 'n behoefte bestaan aan die beskrywing van 'n prakties jiiitvoerbare evaluasietegniek wat objektiewe data van veral die slukproses kan verskaf. sodanige data kan vir beide navorsing en terapeutiese intervensie aangewend word. die doel;van hierdie studie is dus om die leemtes in die kliniese praktyk aan te vul deur die slukpatrone van serebraal gestremdes wetenskaplik te beskryf en die verskillende tipes serebraal gestfemdes met mekaar te vergelyk. metode doelstelljngs: die hoofdoel van die studie is om die slukpatrone van serebraal gestremde kinders te ondersoek. die spesifieke doelstellings is: om te bepaal wat die aard van moontlike slukprobleme by serebraal gestremdes in die oraal-voorbereidende, orale en faringale fases, is; om te bepaal of, en in watter opsigte die slukpatrone van die drie hooftipes serebraal gestremdes, naamlik die spastiese, atetotiese en ataktiese tipes, verskil; om te bepaal wat die invloed van behandeling van oraal motoriese probleme en maturasie van orale funksionering op serebraal gestremde kinders se slukpatrone is deur twee verskillende ouderdomsgroepe serebraal gestremdes te vergelyk. ν a vorsjngsontwerp die ondersoek het die vorm aangeneem van 'n beskrywende opnamestudie waarin tussengroepvergelykings getref is (smit, 1983). proefpersone: daar is drie groepe proefpersone geselekteer, naamlik twee eksperimentele groepe, elk bestaande uit 5 serebraal gestremde proefpersone en een kontrolegroep bestaande uit 2 normale proefpersone. groepe 1 en 2 is leerlinge aan 'n skool vir serebraal gestremdes in pretoria. atetotiese, ataktiese en spastiese kinders is in die groepe opgeneem. die 5 proefpersone in groep 1 het reeds minstens twee jaar terapie ontvang vir voedingsprobleme ten einde die invloed van behandeling op die slukproses te kon bepaal. die proefpersone was tussen die ouderdomme van 11 jaar en 12 jaar ses maande wat verseker het dat anatomiese ontwikkeling van die larinks (wat 'n effek het op sluk) reeds voorgekom het (kennedy & kent, 1985). j die 5 proefpersone in groep 2 was tussen die ouderdomme drie en ses jaar en het nie langer as ses maandebehandeliiig ontvang vir hul slukprobleme nie. sodoende kon die invloed van behandeling van oraal motoriese probleme e h , maturasie van die orale strukture op die slukproses van die proefpersone bepaal word. die proefpersone word in tabelle 1 tot 3 beskryf. τ abel 1. beskrywing van die relevante eienskappe van eksperimentele groep1ά kenmerke ' proefpersone 1 2 3 4 5 geslag van kind manlik manlik manlik vroulik vroulik ouderdom van kind · 12,6 11 12,9 12,2 12,10 tipe serebrale gestremdheid atakties atakties atetoties spasties spasties graad van aantasting ' gemiddeld gemiddeld gemiddeld gemiddeld gemiddeld tydperk van terapie ontvang 2 j r 2,1 jr 2,6 jr 2,3 jr 2 j r · ' intelligensie ondergemiddeld gemiddeld the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 17 videofluorografiese ondersoek van die slukproses by serebraal gestremde kinders tabel 2 beskrywing van die relevante eienskappe van eksperimentele groep2 kenmerke proefpersone 6 7 8 9 10 geslag van kind ouderdom van kind tipe serebrale gestremdheid graa3 van aantasting typerk terapie ontvang manlik 5,5 atakties gemiddeld 7 mnde vroulik 6,1 atetoties gemiddeld 6 mnde vroulik 5,11 spasties gemiddeld 5 mnde vroulik 4,6 spasties gemiddeld 4 mnde vroulik 4,0 atakties gemiddeld 3 mnde intelligensie gemiddeld tabel 3. beskrywing van die relevante eienskappe van die kontrolegroep kenmerke 1 2 geslag van kind vroulik manlik ouderdom van kind 11,4 5,3 intelligensie bo-gemiddeld gemiddeld apparaat en materiaal die volgende apparaat en materiaal is tydens die uitvoering van die eksperiment gebruik: % siemens siregraph ii vir die videofluorografiese opnames. die bestralingsdosis was 141 ma per minuut. die totale blootstellingstyd aan bestraling vir elke kind was ongeveer 45 sekondes; 'n sony u-matic video-opnemer, en 'n sony u-matic kcs 20-videoband, vir die video-opnames van die slukproses; ezhb bariumsulfaatvloeistof vir die proefpersone om te sluk, om sodoende die evaluasie van die slukproses moontlik te maak; evaluasievorm waarvolgens die slukproses by elke proefpersoon geevalueer word (sien aspekte genoem by dataanalise). / prosedure elke proefpersoon is individueel geevalueer. die kinders is geposisioneer in 'n sittende posisie terwyl hulle na vore kyk. 'n laterale beeld van die slukproses is gefotografeer. die deiirligting is volgens die standaardprosedure uitgevoer, terwyl die video-opnemer die beeld aanhoudend opgeneem het (logemann, 1983). geen kopstut is gebruik in die eksperiment nie, aangesien al die proefpersone voldoende kopkontrole gehad het. ezhb bariumsulfaat is gebruik om die beeld wat waargeneem is te verhelder. die farinks is met materiaal gevul. sodoende het die strukture duideliker sigbaar geword en kon die slukproses makliker geevalueer word (haubrich, 1977). elke proefpersoon moes 'n sluk van die bariumsulfaat neem en dit in hul mond hou en dan op bevel sluk. die ondersoeker kon sodoende bepaal of die proefpersone willekeurig 'n slukrefleks kon ontlok. elke proefpersoon het twee keer bariumsulfaat gesluk, wat die blootstellings aan bestraling minder as 1 minuut gemaak het (brogan, et al. 1987). elke sluk is op videoband opgeneem en later aan die hand van kriteria geevalueer deur die ondersoeker en 'n medeondersoeker met gesofistikeerde kennis van die slukproses asook van videofluorografie. data-analise: data-optekening die videoband is vir opleidingsdoeleindes drie maal deurgegaan sodat die posisie van orale strukture asook die fases in die slukproses gei'dentifiseer kon word. dit is gedoen met behulp van 'n sony u-matic redigeerstel. die ondersoeker en die mede-ondersoeker het daarna saam na die videoband gekyk, en met behulp van raam-vir-raam metings, is elke proefpersoon se slukproses geevalueer. moontlike probleme is gei'dentifiseer en daar is weer na die hele slukproses van elke proefpersoon gekyk om die slukprobleme finaal te identifiseer en te klassifiseer. nadat die ondersoeker en mede-ondersoeker ooreenstemming bereik het ten opsigte van klassifisering van die probleme, is daar op 'n optekeningsvorm aangeteken wat normaal en afwykend in die waargenome slukproses voorgekom het, asook die tyd wat dit vir elke persoon geneem het om te sluk. aspekte geevalueer die eerste drie fases van die slukproses is geevalueer, naamlik die oraal-voorbereidende fase, orale fase en die faringale fase. logemann (1983) se beskrywings van elke fase van die slukproses is gebruik om die waargenome slukbewegings van die proefpersone mee te vergelyk, te beskryf en te kategoriseer (le roux, 1988). resultate die resultate van die videofluorografiese analise van die oraal voorbereidende, orale en faringale fases in die slukproses van die drie groepe proefpersone word vervolgens kortliks beskryf en in tabel 4 samevattend weergegee. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 • . , κ a r m le roux, anita van der merwe en brenda louw tabel 4. resultate van eksperimentele groepe 1 en 2. kriteria proefpersone kriteria groep1 groep 2 kriteria 1 2 3 4 5 atakatakatespasspasties ties toties ties ties 1 2 3 4 5 atakatespasspasatakties toties ties ties ties a. oraal voorbereidende fase i. bolus-formasie: kan vloeistof in mond hou kan 'n bolus behou b. orale fase voldoende tong-palatumkontak normale linguale peristalsis geen vasklewing aan harde verhemeltevoldoende anterior-posterior tongbewegings beheerde bolus voldoende velere elevasie voldoende slukrefleks c. faringale fase geen vallekula stasis voeldoende tiroi'ed-elevasie voldoende laringale elevasie voldoende laringale sluiting krikofaringale koordinasie geen statis in piriforme sinusse (links en regs) totale tyd geneem om sluki proses te voltooi (sek.) * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * _ * * * * * * * _ _ * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * 2 3 2 . 2 3 * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * * 3 3 4 3 3 sleutel: * = aanwesig afwesig kontrolegroep by hierdie groep proefpersone het geen afwykings in die slukproses voorgekom nie. die kontrolepersone kon die vloeistof in hul mond hou en so ook die bolusvorm behou. voldoende tongpalatumkontak het voorgekom wat normale linguale peristalsis tot gevolg gehad het. geen vasklewing aan die harde verhemelte, voldoende anterior-posterior tongbeweging en beheerde bolus wat normale sluk veronderstel, het voorgekom. voorts was daar voldoende velere elevasie en 'n normaal ontlokte slukrefleks. met betrekking tot die faringale fase was daar geen vallekula stasis nie en voldoende tiro'ieden-laringale elevasie, sowel as voldoende laringale sluiting en krikofaringale koordinasie is deur hierdie kinders vertoon. laastens het geen stasis, in die piriforme sinusse voorgekom nie.." die totale tyd wat dit geneem het pm die bolus te sluk, was binne die perke van ongeveer twee sekondes wat in die literatuur as normale sluk geklassifiseer word (logemann, 1983; lear, et al. 1965). eksperimentele groep 1 eksperimentele groep 1 wat bestaan uit serebraal gestremde kinders tussen nege en dertien jaar wat reeds vir 'n paar jaar terapie ontvang het, het tvdens die oraal voorbereidende fase geen probleme vertoon nie. elkeen kon die vloeistof in hul monde hou en ook die bolusvorm behou. dit impliseer dat hierdie kinders voldoende mandibulabeweging en mobiliteit van die lippe het om lipsluiting te bewerkstellig. in die orale fase is elke probleem waargeneem. twee proefpersone (4 en 5), albei spastiese kwadruplee, het onvoldoende tongpalatumkontak vertoon. dit het tot gevolg gehad dat daar residu van die vloeistof op hul harde verhemelte agtergebly het. hierdie onvoldoende tongpalatumkontak kan moontlik wees as gevolg van onvoldoende tongelevasie. van riper en emerick (1984) noem dat die spastiese serebraal gestremde as gevolg van hipertonus dit moeilik vind om gelykmatige en gegradeerde bewegings uit te voer. die onvermoe om die bewegings gelykmatig en gegradeerd uit te voer, het ook betrekking op die spraakmeganisme. hierdie onvermoe kan moontlik ook van toepassing gemaak word op die beweging van die tong. die hipertonus be'invloed die kind se vermoe om die tong voldoende op te lig en dus tongpalatumkontak te bewerkstellig. verder het al die proefpersone normale linguale peristalsis en voldoende anterior-posterior tongbeweging getoon en geen vasklewing aan die harde verhemelte het voorgekom nie. 'n voldoende slukrefleks was ook teenwoordig. proefpersoon 4 het onvoldoende vel£re elevasie vertoon. vertraagde velere elevasie kan soms !n onvoldoende velofaringale sluiting tot gevolg he. alhoewel voedsel soms in die nasale holtes kan kom, was dit nie hier die geval nie. al die ander the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) videofluorografiese ondersoek van die slukproses by serebraal gestremde kinders 19 proefpersone het egter voldoende velere elevasie vertoon. in die faringale fase het proefpersone 2 en 3, ataktiese en atetotiese serebraal gestremde kinders onderskeidelik, stasis in die vallekula vertoon. alhoewel die slukrefleks betyds ontlok word en die bolus nie in die vallekula vassteek nie, kan daar residuele vloeistof agterbly in die vallekula nadat die sluk reeds afgehandel is. hierdie residu dui gewoonlik op 'n vermindering in krag van die faringale peristalsis. indien daar slegs in die een kant van die vallekula residu voorkom, dui dit 'n swakheid aan daardie kant aan (logemann, 1983). geen ander proefpersoon het vallekula stasis vertoon nie. voldoende tiro'ieden laringale elevasie asook voldoende laringale sluiting het voorgekom by al die proefpersone in eksperimentele groep 1. die totale tyd wat die proefpersone geneem het om die slukproses, vanaf die oraal voorbereidende na die orale en laastens die faringale fase te voltooi, was ook normaal, naamlik gemiddeld 2,4 sekondes (logemann, 1983). eksperimentele groep 2 hierdie groep hetoor die algemeen meer probleme vertoon as die proefpersone in eksperimentele groep 1. tydens die oraal voorbereidende fase het al die proefpersone daarin geslaag om die vloeistof in hul mond te hou. proefpersone 6, 7, 8 en 9, 'n ataktiese, atetotiese en twee spastiese serebraal gestremdes onderskeidelik, het egter 'n onvermoe vertoon om die bolus te behou. die vloeistof het deur die orale holte versprei en is nie in 'n klein massa bymekaar gehou nie. hierdie onvermoe kan moontlik aan verminderde en beperkte tongbewegings toegeskryf word. in die orale fase het proefpersone 6, 7, 8 en 10 onvoldoende tong-palatumkontak vertoon. soos beskryf by die resultate van groep 1, het daar gevolglik residu van die materiaal aan die proefpersone se harde verhemelte vasgekleef. onvoldoende tongpalatumkontak kan moontlik aan verminderde tongelevasievermoens toegeskryf word (logemann, 1983). ί proefpersone 6, 7 en 8 het abnormale linguale peristalsis vertoon. die versteurde linguale peristalsis is moontlik te wyte aan gedisorganiseerde anterior-posterior beweging van die tong. die onvermoe om die fjjner spraakstruktuurbewegings te bemeester, kan gedisorganiseerde tongbewegings tydens sluk teweegbring (macdonald 8ί chance, 1964). abnormale vasklewing van) die vloeistof aan die harde verhemelte is by proefpersone 6 , 7 , 8 en 10 opgemerk. residu van die vloeistof het aan die verhemelte vasgekleef nadat die sluk reeds afgehandel was. die bolus het verdeel en slegs 'n deel het beweeg na die farinks en die faringale fase van sluk. voldoende anterior-posterior tongbeweging bet die bolus na agter gevoer by al die proefpersone in hierdie groep. proefpersone 6, 7 , 8 en 9 het egter almal 'n onbeheerde bolus vertoon, deurdat 'n deel van die bolus verlore gegaan het as gevolg van onvoldoende tongkontrole. al die proefpersone het in die orale fase voldoende velere elevasie vertoon en die slukrefleks is normaal ontlok. in die faringale fase het proefpersoon 9, 'n spastiese kwadrupleeg, stasis in die vallekula vertoon. hierdie vallekula stasis het by geen ander proefpersoon voorgekom *nie. geen verdere probleem is in die faringale fase gevind by enige van die proefpersone nie. die totale tyd wat dit die proefpersone geneem het om die slukproses te voltooi, was gemiddeld 3,2 sekondes, 0,8 sekondes stadiger as die tyd geneem vir eksperimentele groep 1. hierdie tyd word egter nog nie as abnormaal beskou nie (logemann, 1983). bespreking van resultate die bespreking van die resultate sal gedoen word aan die hand van die doelwitte van die studie. die aard van die slukprobleem by die serebraal gestremde kinders \ uit die beskrywing van die resultate is dit duidelik dat daar wel slukprobleme by serebraal gestremde kinders voorkom. slegs eksperimentele groep 2 het probleme ondervind in die oraal-voorbereidende fase. sommige van die proefpersone het 'n onvermoe vertoon om die bolus te behou. hierdie onvermoe is gekenmerk deur die verspreidirfg van die bolus deur die orale holte. robbins (1985) noem dat afwykende lipfunksies en tongbewegings as gevolg van neuromotoriese aantasting kan resulteer in 'n onvermoe om die bolusvorm te behou in die willekeurige fase van sluk. die versteurde anterior, posterior, laterale en basale linguale bewegings kan ook lei tot spesifieke probleme. verminderde tongpuntsterkte of beperkte beweging van die tong, manifesteer in 'n stadige of oneffektiewe bolusformasie. logemann (1983) noem ook dat serebraal gestremdes wel 'n onvermoe kan vertoon om die bolusvorm te behou. die huidige studie bevestig dus hierdie standpunt. in die orale fase het onvoldoende tongpalatumkontak by ses van die proefpersone voorgekom en kan moontlik aan onvoldoende tongelevasie toegeskryf word (logemann, 1983). die onvoldoende tongbewegings kan ook voorkom as gevolg van die versteurde spiertonus (hiper-, hipoen fluktuerende tonus) wat kenmerkend van hierdie serebraal gestremde kinders is (van riper & emerick, 1984). die abnormale linguale peristalsis wat by drie van die proefpersone voorkom, kan moontlik verband hou met onvoldoende tongpalatumkontak as gevolg van die versteurde spiertonus. die gevolg van die afwykende linguale peristalsis is dan gedisorganiseerde tongbewegings. vasklewing van die vloeistof aan die harde verhemelte hou ook verband met onvoldoende tongbewegings. die tong se onvermoe orti die vloeistof posterior te beweeg, vind plaas as gevolg van hiper-en hipotonus (logemann, 1983). vier proefpersone, almal van eksperimentele groep 2, het 'n onbeheerde bolus gehad. die bolus het deur die mond versprei nadat die persone die vloeistof gesluk het. dit word toegeskryf aan hul onvermoe om tongbewegings te beheer en te koordineer. al die bogenoemde probleme wat in die orale fase voorgekom het, hou verband met mekaar. verminderde tongbewegings, afwykende krag van beweging of'n afwyking in die tydsberekening van die posterior tongelevasie sal resulteer in onbeheerde bewegings van dip bolus deur die orale holte na die farinks voordat die faringale respons gei'nisieer word. die verminderde omvang van posterior linguale beweging sal resulteer in vasklewing aan die harde verhemelte nadat die orale fase reeds afgehandel is (robbins, 1985). dit is egter belangrik om in gedagte te hou dat die orale strukture betrokke by die slukproses, onder andere die mandibula, lippe en tong, nie die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 karin le roux, anita van der merwe e n brenda louw onafhanklik van mekaar funksioneer nie. die slukproses is inderdaad die produk van interaksie tussen strukture. by spesifieke neurologiese afwykings kan unieke oromotoriese patrone resulteer, wat dan die slukproses en spraakproduksie aantas (robbins, 1985). ekedahl, mansson en sandberg (1974) noem dat serebraal gestremde kinders veral probleme ondervind in die orale fase. een van die proefpersone van eksperimentele groep 1 het probleme ondervind met velere elevasie. die vertraagde velere elevasie kan tot gevolg he dat daar onvoldoende velofaringale sluiting plaasvind en vloeistof na die nasale holtes toe kan beweeg. die spastisiteit wat deur van riper en emerick (1984) beskryf word as die gevolg van gelyktydige sametrekking van antagonistiese spiergroepe, kan moontlik die oorsaak wees van onvoldoende velere elevasie. in die faringale fase het drie proefpersone stasis in die vallekula vertoon. vallekula stasis kan moontlik die gevolg van onvoldoende beheer van die bolus wees. die vallekula stasis van hierdie kinders kan te wyte wees aan die konsistensie van die barium wat hul vir die eksperiment moes s l u k dit is 'n dik, stroopagtige vloeistof, wat hul poging om te sluk verder bemoeilik. kennedy en kent (1985) merk ook op dat sommige neurologies aangetaste persone meer probleme ondervind met die sluk van vloeistowwe as vaste stowwe. proefpersoon 1, 'n ataktiese serebraal gestremde kind, het in hierdie fase krikofaringale inkoordinasie getoon. die groep serebraal gestremdes vertoon hoofsaaklik 'n onvermoe om bewegings van die onderskeie spiergroepe te koordineer (hardy, 1983). hierdie bevinding bevestig die resultate van logemann (1983). die tydsduur van die genoemde fases was vir elke individu gemiddeld 2,4 sekondes wat binne die perke van die duur van normale sluk geklassifiseer word (logemann, 1983;-lear et al. 1965). die slukpatrone van die ataktiese, atetotiese en spastiese serebraal gestremdes geen duidelik identifiseerbare slukpatrone kom by die drie tipes serebraal gestremde proefpersone in groep 2 voor nie. tydens die oraal voorbereidende fase vertoon al drie tipes serebraal gestremde proefpersone 'n onvermoe om die bolus te behou. tydens die orale fase vertoon die verskillende tipes serebraal gestremde proefpersone ook ooreenstemmende foutpatrone. onvoldoende tongpalatumkontak, afwykings in linguale peristalsis, vasklewing van die bolus aan die harde verhemelte en 'n swak beheerde bolus kom gesamentlik by die meeste proefpersone voor. proefpersoon 4, wat spasties is, vertoon die minste probleme, naamlik slegs beperkte beheer van die bolus. die aftder spastiese proefpersoon het egter al die genoemde probleme gemeen met die ander tipes serebraal gestremdes. op grond van die beskikbare data wil dit dus voorkom asof individuele faktore soos waarskynlik die graad van aantasting, die slukpatrone van die jonger groep proefpersone bepaal eerder as die tipe serebrale gestremdheid. by groep 2, wat uit die ouer groep proefpersone bestaan, is daar twee interessante verskynsels wat na vore kom. slegs die spastiese groep vertoon residuele probleme in die orale fase en wel met betrekking tot tongpalatumkontak en velere elevasie. verhoogde spiertonus is waarskynlik die oorsaak vir die beperkte omvang van bewegings (darley, aronson & brown, 1975 : 80). die tweede verskynsel wat die data aan die lig bring, is dat slegs ataktiese en atetotiese proefpersone residuele probleme in die faringale fase vertoon. vallekula stasis of krikofaringale wankoordinasie word by drie van die proefpersone waargeneem. geen proefpersoon in die jonger groep het egter sodanige probleme vertoon nie. hierdie tipe probleme kan dus nie as kenmerkend van hierdie twee tipes serebrale gestremdheid beskryf word nie. dit is egter wel betekenisvol dat die ataktiese en atetotiese serebraal gestremdes as 'n groep na vore tree teenoor die spastiese groep aangesien die aard van die letsel by die eerste twee tipes geheel andersoortig en ook meer ooreenstemmend is as die letsel van die spastiese tipe. die serebellum en basale ganglia speel 'n rol by onder andere die koordinasie van beweging, terwyl die motorkorteks geen sodanige rol het nie (darley et al. 1975; eccles, 1977). dit wil dus voorkom asof die tipe serebrale gestremdheid wel 'n rol kan speel by die aard van residuele slukprobleme by ouer serebraal gestremde persone. 'n groter steekproef is egter nodig voordat enige gevolgtrekking in hierdie verband gemaak kan word. die invloed van behandeling en maturasie op slukpatrone uit die resultate is dit duidelik dat daar minder probleme in die slukproses van die groep ouer serebraal gestremde kinders voorkom as in die van die groep jonger kinders. dit kan waarskynlik toegeskryf word aan maturasie van die orale strukture en/of behandeling van die voedingsprobleme. dit is belangrik om te onthou dat as daar met normale ontwikkeling ingemeng word, daardie abnormale komponente distorsie en agterstande in die normale ontwikkelingsproses sal veroorsaak (morris, 1985). die abnormale posturale tonus, abnormale sensoriese integrasie en kompensatoriese bewegingspatrone wat serebraal gestremde kinders ervaar, het ook 'n negatiewe uitwerking op die ontwikkeling van oraal motoriese vaardighede. die ouer serebraal gestremde kinders het egter oor 'η 1 anger tydperk spraakterapie, fisioterapie en arbeidsterapie ontvang vir hul probleme. hierdie behandeling dra by tot stabiliteit en mobiliteit van die hele liggaam maar ook tot die beheer van die orale strukture. ( i die behandeling wat die kinders vir voedingsprobleme by (lie betrokke skool ontvang, is gebaseer op die bobath benadering. die spesifieke tegnieke wkt die terapeute toepas, blyk baie siiksesvol te wees as daar gelet word op die beperkte hoeveelheid probleme wat die groep ouer kinders in hul slukproses vertoon teenoor die van die groep jonger kinders. ^ die groep drietot sesjarige serebraal gestremdes het beduidend meer slukprobleme vertoon as die groep ouer proefpersone. die probleme wat hierdie kinders in die slukproses ondervind, het hoofsaaklik in die oraal voorbereidende en orale fase voorgekom. kenmerkend was dat die beperkte en verminderde tongbewegings vertoon deur hierdie proefpersone, die meeste van die probleme in die betrokke fases veroorsaak. robbins (1985) beskryf die tong as die prim6re bewegingsorgaan tydens die eerste twee fases van die slukproses, asook in spraakproduksie. die tong neem 'n verskeidenheid van posisies tydens spraak en ook die slukproses in. tydens die slukproses (hoofsaaklik die oraal-voorbereidende en orale fase) word daar 'n komplekse patroon van gegradeerde veranderinge in spieraktiwiteit uitgevoer, waartydens fyn kontrole en voldoende krag van die intrinsieke sowel as ekstrinsieke / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) videofluorografiese ondersoek van die slukproses by serebraal gestremde kinders 21 spiere nodig is vir die volvoering van 'n normale slukproses (robbins, 1985). kennedy en kent (1985) verduidelik dat die daling van die larinks tot op die vlak van die vierde tot sewende servikale werwels ook 'n verlenging van die faringale holte veroorsaak. die volwassene se slukproses, vokalisering en asemhaling verskil dus baie van die van 'n baba. daar kan nie presies bepaal word of die verandering oor ouderdom, die gevolg van behandeling of maturasie is nie. beide hierdie faktore is aanwesig by al die proefpersone en verbetering is dus waarskynlik die gevolg van beide maturasie en behandeling. gevolgtrekkings op grond van die resultate van hierdie ondersoek kan die volgende gevolgtrekkings omtrent die proefpersone se slukprobleme gemaak word: dit is duidelik dat daar wel slukprobleme by serebraal gestremde kinders voorkom. die probleme het hoofsaaklik in die orale fase van die slukproses voorgekom. logemann (1983) se bevinding dat meer probleme in die orale fase opgemerk word, ondersteun die resultate van hierdie studie. die aantasting van die orale spiergroepe by die serebraal gestremdes verskil drasties van kind tot kind. hul kan byvoorbeeld 'n onvermoe vertoon om die bolus te behou; die bolus versprei dan deur die orale holte en gedisorganiseerde tongbewegings verhoed dat die kind op 'n normale wyse kan sluk. die totale tyd wat elk van die proefpersone geneem het om die oraal voorbereidende, orale en faringale fases van die slukproses te voltooi, was ongeveer 2,5 sekondes wat logemann (1983) nog as voldoende beskryf. die motoriese koordinasie probleme het dus nie 'n invloed op die duur van die sluk by enige van die twee eksperimentele groepe tot gevolg gehad nie. die jonger groep proefpersone vertoon ooreenstemmende , slukprobleme ongeag die tipe serebrale gestremdheid. by / die ouer groep was daar wel aanduidings dat die tipe serebrale gestremdheid 'n jrol kan speel by residuele slukprobleme. slegs spastiese -proefpersone vertoon probleme in die orale fase en sle'gs ataktiese en atetotiese proefpersone vertoon probleme in die faringale fase. die steekproefgrootte is egter te beperk om enige gevolgtrekkings in hierdie verband te maak. ι i die feit dat die jonger groep soveel meer probleme vertoon het, dui daarop dat die behandeling wat die groep ouer kinders ontvang het vir enige voedingsprobleme 'n invloed op die slukproses uitgeoefen het. moontlik speel maturasie ook 'n rol deurdat dit meer stabiliteit en mobiliteit aan hierdie proefpersone verskaf het in hul orale strukture om dan sonder enige probleme te kan sluk. ι die inligting wat tydens hierdie studie verkry is, het belangrike implikasies vir die evaluering en behandeling van die serebraal gestremde kind se voedingsprobleme. videofluorografie as 'n objektiewe meetinstrument is effektief gei'mplementeer om die aard van hierdie probleme bloot te 16. navorsing op hierdie gebied in suid-afrika 16 egter nog braak alhoewel waardevolle riglyne vir die toepassing verkry is. hierdie studie kan dus as vertrekpunt dien vir verdere navorsing. verwysings alexander, r. prespeech and feeding development. in ε. t. mcdonald (red. ) treating cerebral palsy. austin : pro-ed, 1987. brogan, w. f. , foulner, d. m. en turner, r. a videofluorographic investigation of the position of the tongue prior to palatal repair in babies with cleft lip and palate. cleft palate journal, 24, 336338, 1987. bzoch, k. r. communicative disorders related to cleft lip and palate. boston:little, brown & company, 1979. darley, f. l., aronson, a. e. en brown, j. r. motor speech disorders. philadelphia: w. b. saunders company, 1975. eccles,j.c. the understanding of the brain. new york: mcgraw-hill, 1977. ekedahl, c., mansson, i. en sandberg, n. swaljpwing dysfunction in the brain damaged with drooling. acta otolaryngology, 78, 141149, 1974. hardy, j. cerebral palsy. englewood cliffs: prentice-hall inc. , 1983. haubrich, w. in defense of the radiographic diagnosis of dysphagia. gastrointestinal endoscopy, 23, 214, 1977. kennedy, j. g. en kent, r. d. anatomy and physiology of deglutition and related functions. seminars in speech and language, 6, 257273, 1985. lear, c. s. c., flanagan, j. b. en moorrees, c. f. a. the frequency of deglutition in man. archives of oral biology, 10, 83-99, 1965. le roux, k. 'n ondersoek van die slukproses by serebraal gestremde kinders. ongepubliseerde b. log-verhandeling. universiteit van pretoria, 1988. logemann, j. evaluation and treatment of swallowing disorders. san diego: college-hill press, 1983. love, r. j. , hagerman, e. l. en tiami, e. g. speech performance, dysphagia and oral reflexes in cerebral palsy. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 45, 59-75, 1980. macdonald, ε. t. en chance, b. cerebral palsy. new jersey: prenticehall, 1964. morris, s. e. development implications for the management of feeding problems in neurologically impaired infants. seminars in speech and language, 6, 293-315, 1985. mueller, h. feeding. in n. r. finnie, handling the young cerebral palsied child at home, 2de uitgawe. london: william heinemann medical books, 1976. robbins, j. swallowing and speech production in the neurologically impaired adult. seminars in speech and language, 6, 337-359, 1985. smit, g. l. navorsingsmetodes in die gedragwetenskappe. pretoria: haum, 1983. steenkamp. m. persoonlike onderhoud. pretoria skool vir serebraal gestremdes, pretoria, 1988. steenkamp, m. nuwe ontwikkelings in die behandeling van serebrale gestremdheid. lesing gelewer by die universiteit van pretoria, 1989. van der walt, r. voedingsprobleme. kort kursus, pretoria, 1988. van riper, c. en emerick, l. speech correction: an introduction to speech pathology and audiology. new jersey: prentice hall, 1984. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) do it yourself insertion gain instrument if you have a suitable ibm compatible computer and if you are familiar with it acoustimed can now offer you a "do it yourself' insertion gain instrument. we sell you a kit which you asemble yourself without any special tools. the equipment has all the features of the ha-2000 ii system but is in a less expensive housing and we save on installation costs. any support which you may need is given over the telephone or in our offices saving you thousands of rands on the world's most versatile hearing aid analyzer. features: complex test signals fast pure tone sweep speech weighted signals transients, bursts, continuous signals built in signal synthesizer real time analysis time delay spectometry "prescription" calculations are programmable auto-correlation for noise reduction signal averaging and spectrum averaging rms, peak and crest factor displayed linear response probe microphone data management with sophisticated data base program easy to use acodat programming language word processor with graphics facility mailing list programs invoicing programs calendar/scheduling program no other system offers all these features. write or call for a descriptive booklet. n b . this is a marketing experiment for which we have prepared two instruments. we reserve the right to request that you bring your computer to us for assembly, demonstration and instruction. acoustimed (ρτύ) ltd 3 2 7 bosman building cor. eloff and bree streets johannesburg tel: ( o i l ) 3 3 7 2 9 7 7 / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u d i t i e w e a a n d a g by d i e voorskoolse kind elmarie niemand m . a . ( l o g . ) ( p r e t . ) departement spraakwetenskap, spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria. opsomming daar is met hierdie navorsingsprojek gepoog om 'n duidelike beeld te vorm van ouditiewe aandagsprosessering by die voorskoolse kind. aangesien ouditiewe aandagsprosessering van groot belang is vir die kind wat skool toe gaan is daar op die groep wat in die daaropvolgende jaar skool toe gaan besluit. 'n groep van 64 kinders is geselekteer. daar is gebruik gemaak van 'n gegradeerde ouditiefgerigte ondersoekprogram wat s<5 saamgestel is dat dit wetenskaplik verantwoordbaar is. in sy geheel gesien dui die navorsingsresultate daarop dat die basiese ontwikkelingsproses van ouditiewe aandagsprosessering op hierdie ouderdom (5;2 tot 5;8) voltooi is. 'n verdere evaluering van dieresultate in terme van die uiteengesette doelstellings van die navorsingsprojek dui daarop dat kinders in hierdie ouderdomsgroep nog soms moeite ondervind om aan 'n ouditiewe sei binne omgewingslawaai aandag te skenk. bekende seine verhoog die vermoe om aandag te skenk. daar kon egter nie vir alle aannames wat in die literatuur gemaak word, bewys gevind word nie. summary the object of this research project is to establish a clear picture of the process of auditory attention of the pre-school child. the immense importance of the process of auditory attention for the school-age child guided the decision to use a group of children qualifying for school in the following year. a group of 64 children was selected. a scientifically acceptable graded auditory research programme was used. in general, the research results indicate that the basic development of the process of auditory attention has been completed at the age of 5;2 to 5;8. a further evaluation indicates that some children, however, still have difficulty in attending to an auditory stimulus in the presence of background noise. known stimuli tend to improve the ability to pay attention. all claims in the literature could not be accounted for. aandag is die aspek van ouditiewe persepsie wat waarskynlik met die totale proses van ouditiewe persepsie verband h o u . 1 3 'n studie in die , veld sal dus van waarde wees wanneer daar na oplossings gesoek word vir vraagstukke wat direk met ouditiewe persepsie en dus ook ouditiewe aandag in verband staan. die belang van aandag word ook deur lewis 1 9 beklemtoon deur sy beskouing dat aandag nie slegs verband hou met ouditiewe persepsie nie, maar dat die toetsing van aandagsvermoens ook van waarde kan wees by die identifikasie van kortikale betrokkenheid en patologie. wanneer daar gepoog word om ouditiewe aandag te definieer is dit moontlik om hedendaagse beskouings in een omvattende definisie te vervat vanwee die groot mate van oorvleueling wat voorkom. so 'n definisie kan soos volg lui: die funksie van aandag is om instelling van die organisme ten opsigte van die sein te verbeter aangesien dit lei tot die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 elmarie niemand aktiewe gemoeidheid met die sein. deur die proses van aandag word inligting verkry deur die inkomende stimuli te filtreer en dan te bepaal watter gedeelte van die inkomende stimuli die sentrale prosesseerder in beslag sal neem op 'n gegewe oomblik. relevante inligting word dus uit die omgewing geselekteer en geprosesseer. in die literatuur word ook begrippe gevind wat baie nou verwant is aan aandag en ook in baie gevalle aandag impliseer. hierdie begrippe is konsentrasie, waaksaamheid, seleksie, soektog, aktivering en instelling.9 dit is dus duidelik dat aandag nie stereotiep van aard is nie, maar dat dit 'n integrale deel van die persepsieproses is. dit lei tot die verwagting dat daar verskillende vorms van aandag sal wees. die verskillende vorms van aandag sluit primere aandag; sekondere aandag; tersiere aandag; aktiewe aandag; passiewe aandag; eksterne, interne, sensoriese en begripsvormende aandag; vernoude aandag; 1 2 selektiewe a a n d a g ; 2 0 fokusaandag5 en ewewigtige aandag in. wat veral hier van belang is, is dat die verskillende vorms van aandag, behalwe vir tersiere, passiewe en ewewigtige aandag, ook tot 'n mindere of meerdere mate oor 'n selektiewe eienskap beskik. hierdie verwantskap wat onderling bestaan, dui ook daarop dat daar waarskynlik spesifieke eienskappe gevind word wanneer die proses van aandag ontleed word. die eienskappe van aandag betrek kontrole, 1 4 die leerproses, 1 8 die inkomende sein, 2 1 die wakkertoestand,1 6 verskillende modaliteite,2 konsentrasie, persoonlikheid,1 2 die sekondere stimulus,2 2 persepsie en integrasie. hieruit kan die belang van aandag as deel van die totale persepsieproses afgelei word, 'n ander aspek wat van belang is, maar tot nog toe baie min aandag geniet het, is die ontwikkeling van aandag. die waarde van die beperkte inligting tot die leser se beskikking le daarin dat dit aandui dat die kind op 'n skoolgaande ouderdom oor 'n goed ontwikkelde ouditiewe prosesseringsisteem beskik. daar is verskillende benaderings tot die aspek van aandag waarvan die mees aanvaarbare waarskynlik die neuro-psigologiese benadering 2 3 is aangesien die rol van die sentrale sisteem ook in berekening gebring word. die doel van hierdie studie is om 'n beeld te vorm van ouditiewe aandagsprosessering soos gepresenteer deur die normale voorskoolse kind. eksperiment doelstellings 1. doelstellings met betrekking tot maskeringsvariante die eerste doel van die eksperiment is om te bepaal of kinders, net voordat hulle skool toe gaan, wel aandag kan skenk aan 'n ouditiewe sein in die teenwoordigheid van omgewingslawaai. 'n tweede doel is om te bepaal of maskering by die normale kind tot gevolg sal he dat daar 'n verskerping in aandag sal wees. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditiewe aandag 21 'η derde doel is daarop gemik om vas te stel of daar 'n betekenisvolle verskil tussen maskering deur middel van wyebandgeraas (pienkruis) en maskering deur middel van 'n roesemoes van stemme bestaan. dit sal dus van akademiese waarde wees om hierdie aspek by die navorsing in te sluit. 2. doelstellings met betrekking tot die sein die eerste doel is om vas te stel of 'n meer betekenisvolle sein, byvoorbeeld 'n klokkie of 'n trom, makliker aandag ontlok en behou as 'n nie-betekenisvolle sein, byvoorbeeld 'n suiwertoon (500 hz of 4 000 hz). 'n tweede doel is om te bepaal of daar 'n verskil gemaak word tussen klankseine en spraakseine wat betref die mate waarin daar aandag daaraan geskenk word. 'n derde doel het te doen met 'n belangrike aspek, naamlik bekendheid wat deur yusson et a l 2 4 beklemtoon word. binne dieselfde subtoets word gebruik gemaak van 'n bekende sein (trom) teenoor 'n onbekende sein (suiwertoon: 500 hz). 3. doelstellings met betrekking tot intensiteit die doel hier is om te bepaal of dit wel so is dat harder klanke of te wel seine met 'n hoer intensiteit makliker aandag trek as klanke wat net op die gehoordrempel val. 4. doelstellings met betrekking tot die intensiteitsverhouding tussen die maskering en die sein die doel van die eksperiment sal ook wees om te bepaal of daar beduidende verskille voorkom by die vermoe om aandag te skenk indien die maskeringslawaai harder as die sein is, gelyk aan die sein is, of sagter as die sein is. 5. doelstellings met betrekking tot die kompleksiteit van die toetsmateriaal die doel hier sal wees om te bepaal of kompleksiteit van die toetsmateriaal 'n invloed uitoefen op die ouditiewe aandagsvermoe van die jong kind. volgens johnson en toppino8 en jordaan et al 9 bestaan daar by die jong kind die neiging om voorkeur te gee aan minder komplekse seine. die eksperiment sal dus gegradeer word. daar sal aanvanklik slegs van die proefpersoon verwag word om aandag te skenk aan 'n klanksein en 'n spraaksein te midde van maskering. daarna word die opdrag meer kompleks deurdat visuele komponente (voorwerpe en prente) ook gei'nkorporeer word en die ouditiewe sein by die visuele sein gepas moet word. hierna word die opdrag verder gekompliseer deurdat van die proefpersoon verwag word om die spraaksein te identifiseer en te reproduseer. die laaste opdrag vereis prosessering van die boodskap asook 'n motoriese handeling na aanleiding van die geprosesseerde boodskap. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 elmarie niemand ή. d o e l s t e l l i n g s met betrekking t o t geslag 'n verdere doel van die eksperiment sal wees om te bepaal of daar enige verskille bestaan tussen seuns en dogters van die ouderdom 5;2 tot 5;8 wat betref ouditiewe aandagsvermoe. 7. doelstellings met betrekking tot gedragspatrone die doel hier is om gedragspatrone waar te neem wat kan dien om die resultate verder toe te lig. aspekte wat hier in ag geneem word is verveling, 'n algemene onvermoe om sekere toetsitems te bemeester, die nodigheid om bo en behalwe die aanvanklike opdrag ook tussenin met die proefpersoon te kommunikeer om motivering te versterk en reaksietyd. proefpersone kriteria vir die seleksie van proefpersone seleksie het volgens die volgende kriteria geskied: om 'n verteenwoordigende beeld van die ouditiewe aandagsvermoens van die proefpersone te verkry, was dit nodig dat seleksie so sal geskied, dat elke area van die stadsgebied van pretoria verteenwoordiging sal geniet. daar is in oorleg met 'n statistikus besluit om 100 proefpersone te selekteer. die ondersoek word so gerig dat die vermoe om aandag te skenk direk gekoppel word aan die klaskamersituasie. die proefpersone is dus so geselekteer dat dit die groep wat die daaropvolgende jaar toegelaat word tot graad i, betrek. hierdie groep is ook verder geskik vir seleksie aangesien daar aangeneem kan word dat die proses van ouditiewe aandagsprosessering reeds op hierdie ouderdom (5;2 tot 5; 8) ten voile ontwikkel i s . 1 7 die moontlikheid van 'n psigo-neurologiese disfunksie moes uitgegeskakel word aangesien ondervinding in die praktyk geleer het dat kinders wat as sodanig geklassifiseer word, meesal leerprobleme presenteer en ook aandagsprobleme ondervind. so 'n proefpersoon sal dus nie geskik wees vir die studie nie. die proefpersone moet ook oor 'n normale intelligensie beskik aangesien dit nie bekend is in hoe 'n mate 'n subnormale intelligensie die aandagsvermoe van die kind sal bei'nvloed nie. die selfhandhawing van die proefpersoon is ook van belang aangesien 'n infantiele emosionele toestand die eksperimentele prosedure nadelig sal bei'nvloed. , ζ 7 die mediese geskiedenis van die proefpersoon moet ook in ag geneem word aangesien aspekte soos epilepsie,6 medikasie,1 gedurige oorinfeksies en mangelinfeksies 'n belangrike rol speel in die afname van die aandagsvermoe. dit is ook belangrik dat die proefpersone oor 'n normale gehoorvermoe beskik aangesien die eksperiment ouditief aangebied word. aangesien die verskille, indien enige, tussen seuns en dogters met betrekking tot ouditiewe aandag bepaal moet word, moet die seleksie the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditiewe aandag 23 van proefpersone sodanig wees dat albei geslagte verteenwoordig word. aangesien die literatuur nie besliste onderskeid maak met betrekking tot geslag nie, is seuns en dogters op 'n toevallige basis geselekteer. die seleksie van proefpersone die proefpersone is geselekteer uit verskeie kleuterskole binne die stadsarea van pretoria. daar is op kleuterskole besluit vir die volgende redes: — proefpersone is makliker bekombaar; — 'n meer betroubare beeld met betrekking tot, byvoorbeeld, intelligensie, sosiale aanpasbaarheid, ensovoorts, kan verkry word deur middel van vraelyste wat onderskeidelik deur die ouers en die onderwyseres voltooi moes word; — 'n verteenwoordigende monster van die populasie van pretoria word so verseker. om te verseker dat die hele stadsgebied van pretoria ingesluit word, is die stad in 5 blokke verdeel waaruit 2 kleuterskole elk geselekteer is. daar is bepaal dat die groep wat in die daaropvolgende jaar (1980, in die geval van die navorsing) skool toe sou gaan tussen die ouderdomme 5;2 tot 5;8 val. alle kinders by die 10 kleuterskole in hierdie ouderdomsgroep is aanvanklik in die navorsing ingesluit as potensiele proefpersone. om 'n oorsigtelike beeld van die kind se algemene ontwikkeling, intelligensie, emosionele ryping, gedrag, aandagsvermoe en mediese geskiedenis te verkry, is afsonderlike vraelyste aan beide die kleuterskoolonderwyseres en ouers vir voltooiing gegee. hierdie vraelyste moes dien om proefpersone wat nie vir hierdie studie geskik is nie, te identifiseer, byvoorbeeld 'n geskiedenis van leerprobleme in die familie. die vraelyste is ook so opgestel dat belangrike aspekte, soos byvoorbeeld intelligensie op beide vraelyste voorkom sodat daar ook 'n mate van kontrole tussen die twee vraelyste is. nadat die gegewens verkry uit die vraelyste gekontroleer is, is moontlike proefpersone geselekteer. hierdie proefpersone is onderwerp aan identifikasie-oudiometrie11 om te bepaal of hulle oor normale gehoor beskik. hierdie siftingstoetse is by die kleuterskole uitgevoer waar die lokaal so gekies is dat omgewingslawaai sodanig beperk is dat 'n normaalhorende persoon die toetstoon wel by 20 db hoor. 7 alle kinders wat ook oor 'n normale gehoor beskik is hierna ingesluit as proefpersone in die eksperiment. daar is uiteindelik van 64 proefpersone gebruik gemaak; 32 seuns en 32 dogters. materiaal en apparaat hier kan 'n tweeledige verdeling gemaak word, naamlik eerstens, die apparaat en materiaal wat van toepassing was by die seleksie van die die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 elmarie niemand proefpersone en tweedens, apparaat en materiaal wat betrekking het op die eksperiment self. 1. materiaal en apparaat vir die seleksie van proefpersone vraelyste twee vraelyste is saamgestel: een vir voltooiing deur die ouers en 'n tweede vir voltooiing deur die onderwyseresse. gehoorsifting hier is gebruik gemaak van 'n draagbare madsen-oudiometer, model ob 40 (iso, 1964). 2. materiaal en apparaat vir die uitvoer van die eksperiment maskering by die keuse van maskering is dit nodig om die aspek van integrasie in gedagte te hou. dit word as belangrik beskou aangesien die parameters van die sein bei'nvloed kan word en dus 'n verkeerdelike of onewe beeld kan gee. daar is besluit op pienkruis omdat dit 'n bree spektrum het en op 'n roesemoes van stemme aangesien dit baie nou verband hou met die omgewingslawaai wat in die klaskamersituasie gevind word. toetsmateriaal die toets is in 5 subtoetse verdeel. in die eerste 4 subtoetse is gebruik gemaak van klank of spraakseine wat kontrasteer deurdat dit hoofsaaklik hoe of lae frekwensies betrek. die 5e subtoets behels verbale opdragte. • * samestelling van die toetsmateriaal en maskering dit is met die hulp van die wetenskaplike navorsing en nywerheidse raad (wnnr) (departement akoestiek) saamgestel. die maskering en die stimulussein is so vermeng dat die sein en die maskering eerstens gelyk is aan mekaar, daarna is die sein 10 db sagter as die maskering en in die laaste instansie, 10 db hoer as die maskering. elke subtoets is 12 keer herhaal. tabel i: 'n voorstelling van die vermenging van die stimulussein en die maskering / 40 db bo die gehoordrempel op die gehoordrempel pienkruis stemme pienkruis stemme sein in db 1 0 2 0 0 i o 2 0 ;o 1 0 2 0 0 1 0 2 0 0 maskering in klankdrukpeil 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 0 1 0 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditiewe aandag 25 die rede vir die herhaling van die pienkruis en die stemme is dat die eksperiment eers 40 db bo die gehoordrempel (60 db) uitgevoer word, en daarna op die gehoordrempel (20 db — na aanleiding van die gehoorsifting) om te bepaal of 'n verhoogde intensiteit aandagsvermoe verbeter. die tydsintervalle tussen elke toetsitem is vooraf ewekansig geselekteer en tydens die opname by die wnnr met behulp van 'n stophorlosie gekontroleer. daar is ook 'n kalibrasiesein op die band aangebring sodat dit gekoppel kan word aan 'n oudiometer en die nodige verstellings (van 60 db na 20 db) met behulp van die oudiometer gedoen kan word. apparaat die apparaat wat uiteindelik in die eksperiment gebruik is, is 'n nagra iv-bandmasjien, 'n oopspoelband waarop die toetsmateriaal is, 'n maico ma 24-oudiometer (iso, 1964), oorfone en twee aangrensende klankdigte lokale. die bandmasjien is aan die oudiometer gekoppel en sodoende kon die toets binouraal en streng gekontroleerd aangebied word. addisionele apparaat is gebruik tydens subtoets 3 waar die proefpersoon die voorwerp of prent moes uitwys. vir die doel is 'n trom, 'n klokkie en prente van 'n wiel en 'n bad op 'n tafeltjie voor die proefpersoon geplaas. die volgorde waarin die items geplaas is, was sodanig dat dit nie dieselfde volgorde is waarin die sein aangebied word nie. daar is deurgaans gepoog om in soverre dif moontlik was die eksperiment so saam te stel dat dit verantwoordbaar was in terme van wetenskaplike akkuraatheid. eksperimentele prosedure die nagra-bandmasjien is aan die oudiometer gekoppel. die oudiometer is met behulp van die kalibrasiesein op die band gekalibreer (vu-meterlesing = 0) sodat lesings op die oudiometer werklike lesings sou wees. elke subtoets is deur die opdrag, wat deur die toetsafnemer direk gegee is, voorafgegaan. daarna is die subkategorie as 'n geheel eers teen 60 db en dan teen 20 db aangebied. daar is besluit dat die eksperiment binouraal aangebied gaan word aangesien dit die alledaagse situasie beter verteenwoordig as 'n monourale sein. dit sluit ook die aspek van lateraliteit uit, met ander woorde, dat in die geval van 'n digotiese luistertaak, die boodskap in die regteroor meer akkuraat weergegee w o r d . 1 5 proefpersone is indiwidueel gesien. die toets is aaneenlopend aangebied en geen pouse is gemaak tussen subtoetse nie. uitputting is voorkom deurdat opdragte voor elke subtoets gegee is en sodoende word die toets onderbreek. tydsintervalle tussen die items van die subtoetse word gevarieer om te verseker dat daar nie 'n reaksiepatroon vasgele word nie, byvoorbeeld sein (3 sek.) sein (3 sek.) sein. 'n aspek die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 elmarie niemand o p ge ho or dr em pe l | st em m e (2 0 db ) | ca •a ο 27 24 24 29 26 26 25 30 31 31 30 32 29 31 29 32 26 27 29 27 31 30 30 29 26 30 29 31 31 31 30 32 32 32 32 32 32 32 30 31 o p ge ho or dr em pe l | st em m e (2 0 db ) | 10 d b 3 * 8 oo oo in cn 25 26 26 31 16 20 23 24 20 25 19 24 9 13 19 24 ι s ι | 00 (s | 5 3 16 18 21 25 23 21 o p ge ho or dr em pe l | st em m e (2 0 db ) | 20 d b 22 20 27 28 18 22 22 29 f) f) f) ο ο on 00 m m (ν (ν 24 27 24 28 32 31 30 28 7 16 20 21 20 22 26 30 30 31 32 32 32 31 31 30 o p ge ho or dr em pe l | pi en kr ui s (2 0 db ) 30 d b 00 9\ © ο rs η λ η σ\ oo ο\ md cs cs cs (ν 30 32 30 32 31 31 29 32 24 26 32 30 32 30 30 30 28 30 27 28 32 31 26 30 32 32 32 32 32 32 31 31 o p ge ho or dr em pe l | pi en kr ui s (2 0 db ) 10 d b η η « η — η η ο in ν cs ( ν 27 30 22 23 28 29 22 27 . 19 24 11 13 20 20 21 23 w 00 ό τ2 | ^ 19 21 21 21 14 20 23 28 o p ge ho or dr em pe l | pi en kr ui s (2 0 db ) 20 d b 22 20 28 29 26 27 28 29 31 31 30 32 30 32 32 32 ί 1 0 10 25 27 31 32 29 32 17 21 22 20 27 26 17 21 31 31 32 31 27 31 31 32 40 d b b o ge ho or dr em pe l st em m e (6 0 db ) 7 0 db 22 27 28 28 24 30 27 27 29 32 31 32 31 32 29 32 21 24 29 31 31 32 29 31 17 19 27 27 30 30 27 29 31 32 32 32 31 31 31 32 40 d b b o ge ho or dr em pe l st em m e (6 0 db ) 50 d b a ' λ r ο φ η ο cn (ν 29 28 28 30 16 24 27 30 24 28 21 22 15 16 23 24 π ι i ι ν β ι ι r 40 d b b o ge ho or dr em pe l st em m e (6 0 db ) 60 d b * ΐ ττ γ ts η η μ γ4 ν «) γι μ ν ν 28 32 30 32 30 32 31 32 22 26 25 29 24 30 26 28 5 7 27 26 30 28 11 16 20 25 32 32 27 20 30 32 40 d b b o ge ho or dr em pe l pi en kr ui s (6 0 db ) 70 d b 30 29 26 30 27 29 29 28 31 32 31 32 31 31 30 32 tβ on 00 γ ι ν μ «η ο γ-υ-ι cs ĉ cs (ν 20 25 27 25 30 30 26 28 31 32 32 32 25 18 29 32 40 d b b o ge ho or dr em pe l pi en kr ui s (6 0 db ) 50 d b 18 15 14 9 18 15 21 21 so ο ψ* 00 η η η *ν α ο co ο ν η μ μ 17 16 7 10 14 12 18 19 η β m η » β | v) v) νο tĉ 00 ·<3· 40 d b b o ge ho or dr em pe l pi en kr ui s (6 0 db ) 60 d b 19 21 26 23 18 19 26 24 32 32 30 32 29 30 30 31 17 24 28 28 25 27 20 21 13 18 19 21 19 14 8 9 13 17 11 15 6 7 28 31 40 d b b o ge ho or dr em pe l m as k. se in η η η ^ η ν m ^ η ν ρί ^ οι π ^ / / η μ rt ^ se un s = 32 d og te rs = 3 2 t ot aa l = 64 su bt oe ts 1 su bt oe ts 2 su bt oe ts 3 su bt oe ts 4 su bt oe ts 5 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditiewe aandag 27 wat nie gekontroleer is nie, is die feit dat toetsiterns altyd in dieselfde volgorde aangebied is en die moontlikheid van leer nie uitgeskakel word nie. daar is in 'n klankdigte kamer gewerk en aangesien dit vir 'n kind bedreigend kan wees, is 'n ouer toegelaat om by die proefpersoon te sit tydens die eksperiment. die ouers is so geplaas dat hy/sy buite die gesigsveld van die proefpersoon was om te voorkom dat die proefpersoon se aandag afgetrek word. alvorens die toetsafnemer na die aangrensende klankdigte kamer gegaan het, is die prosedure van die eksperiment aan die proefpersoon verduidelik. daar is ook aan die proefpersoon gevra om die voorwerpe en prente op die tafel te identifiseer om te verseker dat die proefpersoon daarmee vertroud is. hierdie voorbereidende prosedure is as belangrik beskou aangesien daar goeie rapport met die kind opgebou moes word. daar moes ook verseker word dat die proefpersoon die prosedure van die eksperiment goed verstaan. soos reeds genoem is die opdragte vir die verskillende subtoetse direk gegee en is dit nie op die band geplaas nie. dit was om die toetsafnemer die geleentheid te gee om kontak met die proefpersoon te behou. dit is belangrik aangesien daar van voorskoolse kinders gebruik gemaak is. 'n toetsvorm is opgestel en die reaksies van die proefpersone is as korrek ( v ) of foutief (x) aangedui. hierdie gegewens is in tabel ii weergegee. .resultate die ontleding van die resultate is gedoen met behulp van die tegniek van regressie-analise met skynveranderlikes. 'n samevattende beeld van die resultate word in tabel iii weergegee. uit hierdie resultate kan die volgende afgelei word: aspekte wat ouditiewe aandagsprosessering nie beinvloed nie daar is slegs 3 aspekte wat deurgaans geen verandering in die respons van die proefpersone meebring nie. hierdie aspekte is: — die aanbieding van die toetsmateriaal op die gehoordrempel; — die wisselwerking tussen die sein en maskering waar die sein en die maskering gelyk is aan mekaar; — geslag. aspekte wat die vermoe om aandag te skenk laat afneem hier is 6 aspekte betrokke. die afleiding kan dus gemaak word dat daar tog op hierdie ouderdom (5;2 tot 5;8) aspekte is wat ouditiewe aandagsprosessering bemoeilik. hierdie aspekte is: — die seine wat hoofsaaklik lae frekwensies betrek; — 'n hoe frekwensiesein, asook die opdrag "vryf jou neus"; die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 elmarie niemand i > ο .a u •ό μ u 2 3 i/i u un υ ή c λ > λ u εβ β c λ > ω ω χι υ τ3 c ω 13 > υ ε λ <*> -j ω α •c « •ό cq ό ο co ό co ό co ό 8 α ο ο. ο η χ s)30)qn$ « γ7γ μ 23 ζ sjsojqns υ a ° g s f £ sjaojqns §• g a 1 3 .1 § ζ ·° i * 8 a s >. ω p ο j< s3 > > ρ s)30)qns ς sjaojqns the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditiewe aandag 29 — wanneer die toetsitems en maskering 40 db bo die gehoordrempel (wat vir die studie as 20 db aangeneem is) aangebied word en die proefpersoon die toetsitem moet herhaal of 'n motoriese opdrag moet uitvoer na aanleiding van die sein; — pienkruis as maskering wanneer klankseine sowel as spraakseine gebruik word; hierdie invloed word verder vergroot indien dit in wisselwerking met 'n sein teen +10 db ten opsigte van die maskering is; — stemme as maskering waar slegs van klankseine gebruik gemaak word; — 'n wisselwerking tussen stemme as maskering en die sein teen 1 0 db ten opsigte van die maskering by klankseine sowel as verbale opdragte; — die aanbieding van die sein teen 1 0 db ten opsigte van die maskering. aspekte wat die vermoe om aandag te skenk laat toeneem die aspekte wat hier aangedui word, is soos volg: — die aanbieding van die sein teen +10 db ten opsigte van maskering in die subtoetse waar die sein herhaal moes word en waar 'n motoriese handeling uitgevoer moes word na aanleiding van die sein; joj as sein by die verbale herhaling waarskynlik omdat dit die enigste betekenisvolle klank is. met hierdie resultate beskikbaar kan die oorspronklike doelstellings in oenskou geneem word en die resultate kan daarop van toepassing gemaak word. bespreking van die resultate aan die hand van die doelstellings die invloed van maskering dit wil uit die resultate voorkom asof maskering wel aandag be'invloed en dit skyn asof kinders van die ouderdom 5;2 tot 5;8 nog nie in alle gevalle kan aandag skenk in die teenwoordigheid van omgewingslawaai nie. wanneer van klankseine as stimulus gebruik gemaak word be'invloed stemme as maskering die response sodanig dat 'n betekenisvolle afname in korrekte response waargeneem is. sodra daar ook 'n visuele sein bygevoeg word en die stimulussein beide uit klanke en spraak bestaan, verlaag pienkruis die aantal korrekte response betekenisvol. hier is ook gevind dat maskering in wisselwerking met die aanbieding van die sein teen 1 0 db en +10 db ten opsigte van die maskering die aantal korrekte response betekenisvol laat afneem. hier is dus heelwat teenstrydighede. die afleiding wat gemaak kan word, is dat omgewingslawaai wel op hierdie ouderdom (5;2 tot 5;8) nog 'n invloed uitoefen. daar is egter heelwat gevalle waar dit nie betekenisvolle invloed uitoefen nie. indien northern en d o w n s 1 7 se aanname dat die ouditiewe sisteem se ontwikkeling op 4;0 voltooi is, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) elmarie niemand 30 l ria 1 0 simon 2 1 en andere, se aannames dat daar 'n verdere'verfymng'van die ontwikkeling plaasvind ook aanvaar kan d sou eese kon word dat die vermoe om aandag te skenk binne η omeewingslawaai waarskynlik deel van die verfyningsproses is, aangesien die groep kinders wat gebruik is in die ouderdomsgroep 5;2 tot 5;8 val. daar kon nie enige bewys gevind word in die geval van hierdie o u d e r d o m s g r o e p (5;2 tot 5; 8), vir die bewering van jordaan et al 9 dat maskering aandag verskerp nie. geen verskil kon gevind word tussen die gebruik van pienkruis en stemme as maskering nie. die invloed van die sein daar kon geen bewys gevind word dat meer betekenisvolle seine, byvoorbeeld 'n klokkie of 'n trom teenoor suiwertone, 'n beter respons tot gevolg het nie. dit wil dus voorkom asof betekenis nie meer op hierdie ouderdom (5;2 tot 5;8) vir die herkenning van seine belangrik is nie. dit blyk ook dat daar nie onderskeid gemaak word tussen klank en spraakseine nie. frekwensie speel wel in 'n mate 'n rol aangesien daar swakker respons verkry is op lae frekwensieseine as in die geval van hoe frekwensieseine. die bekendheid met die stimulus is 'n aspek wat wel die vermoe om aandag te skenk be'invloed. die feit dat subtoets 2 so min inligting verskaf, word toegeskryf aan die aspek van bekendheid. daar word slegs van die proefpersone verwag om 'n vrouestem ten opsigte van maskering te herken, en daarop te reageer. hulle word daagliks aan 'n vrouestem (kleuterskoolonderwyseres) blootgestel en dit is dus baie bekend aan hulle. in subtoets 3 vaar die proefpersone betekenisvol swakker waar die woord ml/ as stimulus gebruik word teenoor die woord /bat/. in die alledaagse lewe is die proefpersone waarskynlik meer blootgestel aan die woord /bat/ as die woord /vil/, aangesien die woord /bat/ 'n huishoudelike woord is. in subtoets 4 is daar 'n betekenisvolle verbetering in die aantal korrekte response by die aanbieding van die spraakklank /o/. daar word in die kleuterskole gebruik gemaak van die geluide wat diere maak en av is die klank wat aan 'n vark verbind word. dit is waarskynlik waarom die proefpersone so goed daarop reageer. 'n verdere aspek wat daarop dui dat bekendheid met die stimulus belangrik is, is die feit dat alle opdragte in subtoets 5 behalwe die opdrag "vryf jou neus" waarskynlik' bekend is aan die proefpersone. hulle vaar daii ook betekenisvol swakker wanneer die opdrag "vryf jou neus" aangebied word. die invloed van die intensiteit van die aanbieding daar word gevind dat daar 'n betekenisvolle afname in respons voorkom by subtoetse 4 en 5, waar net van spraakseine gebruik gemaak is, wanneer hierdie seine teen 60 db aangebied word. dit is belangrik om in gedagte te hou dat die maskering ook teen 60 db the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditiewe aandag 31 aangebied is, en dus die reaksie van die proefpersone mag be'invloed. daar is egter geen bewys gevind vir die stelling van jordaan et al9 dat seine met 'n hoer intensiteit makliker aandag trek as seine wat op die gehoordrempel val nie. die invloed van die intensiteitsverhouding tussen die maskering en die sein hier blyk dit deurgaans dat indien die stimulussein 10 db swakker as die maskering is, daar 'n betekenisvolle afname in die aantal korrekte response van die proefpersone is. 'n verdere aspek wat 'n afname teweegbring is die wisselwerking tussen die aanbieding van die sein teen 1 0 db ten opsigte van die maskering en die gebruik van 'n roesemoes van stemme as maskering. sodra die sein 10 db bo die maskering aangebied word, is daar 'n betekenisvolle verbetering in die aantal korrekte response van die proefpersone en wel met betrekking tot spraakseine. hierdie resultate moet in ag geneem word wanneer kinders wat in hierdie ouderdomsgroep (5;2 tot 5;8) val, blootgestel word aan konvensionele sein-tot-ruistoetse. dit behoort interessant te wees om die resultate van sulke toetse te vergelyk met die resultate van hierdie studie. die invloed van die kompleksiteit van die toetsmateriaal daar kon geen bewys gevind word vir die feit dat meer komplekse seine aandagsvermoens be'invloed nie. by die bespreking van die opstel van die eksperimentele materiaal is genoem dat daar gegradeer is wat betref die moeilikheidswaarde van die toetsmateriaal. indien kompleksiteit wel 'n rol speel sou die verwagting dus wees dat die aantal korrekte response moet afneem namate die toets vorder. dit het egter nie gebeur nie en vandaar die afleiding dat dit geen invloed uitoefen nie. die moontlikheid dat die moeilikheidswaarde nie genoegsaam gegradeer is nie, kan nie uitgeskakel word nie. die rol van geslag hier kon deurgaans geen verskille gevind word nie en dit blyk dus dat dit geen invloed uitoefen op aandagsvermoens van die kind op die ouderdom 5;2 tot 5;8 nie. die waarneming van gedragspatrone wat die resultate mag be'invloed hierdie waarnemings is tydens toetsing gedoen en 'n aantekening is gemaak wanneer enige gedragspatroon voorgekom het wat die toetsresultate mag be'invloed. daar is slegs in twee gevalle verveling opgemerk. die proefpersone het verder deurgaans stil gesit en aandagtig geluister na die opdrag en daarna na die stimuli. daar is gepoog om die duur van die toets te beperk om verveling uit te skakel. dit blyk geslaagd te wees. geeneen van die gekose toetsitems was te gekompliseerd nie en die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 elmarie niemand opdragte is ook dadelik begryp. die moeilikheidswaarde blyk dus ook nie 'n faktor te wees wat die resultate kon bei'nvloed nie. dit was met die jonger groep (5;2) soms nodig om met die oorskakeling van die aanbieding teen 60 db na 20 db 'n bietjie aanmoediging te gee in die vorm van verbale beloning. dit was veral tussen die aanbiedings van toetse 4 en 5 nodig. dit was egter nie deurgaans gedoen nie en slegs wanneer dit wou voorkom asof daar 'n algemene afname in motivering was. geeneen van die proefpersone het moeite ondervind om te reageer binne die tydperke wat daargestel was nie. die tydperk gelaat tussen die verskillende items van die subtoetse was dus heeltemal voldoende. gevolgtrekkings uit die literatuur het dit geblyk dat die aspek van ouditiewe aandagsvermoens op die ouderdom van 4;0 sodanig ontwikkel is, dat daar slegs verdere verfyning van die bestaande prosesse voorkom tussen die ouderdomme 5;0 tot 1 4 ; 0 . 1 7 ' 1 9 wanneer die resultate verkry uit die eksperiment van hierdie studie aan die stelling gemeet word kan die stelling aanvaar word, mits die invloed wat maskering op aandag uitoefen beskou word as 'n verdere verfyning van die proses. daar is egter nie deurgaans gevind dat maskering in alle gevalle die vermoe om aandag te skenk nadelig be'invloed nie aangesien sommige proefpersone wel goed gereageer het ten spyte van die maskering. daarom word 'n verdere afleiding gemaak dat, indien dit 'n verfyningsproses is, nie alle kinders op skoolgaande ouderdom hierdeur be'invloed sal word nie. hierdie is dan ook waarskynlik 'n aspek wat aandag moet geniet wanneer besluite aangaande skoolrypheid geneem moet word. daar sal bepaal moet word of die kind wel kan aandag skenk binne 'n omgewingslawaai aangesien dit 'n belangrike element is vanwee die hoe peil van omgewingslawaai in die klaskamer van veral die jonger groepe (gr i en gr i i ) . 1 9 op die ouderdom 5;2 tot 5;8 is die kind ook nog baie aangewys op die bekendheid van die materiaal waarna geluister word. hierdie vermoe om aandag te skenk aan relatief minder bekende seine is dus waarskynlik ook deel van die verfyningsproses. dit is weer eens 'n aspek waarna opgelet moet word wanneer besin word oor die feit dat die kind skoolgereed is, al dan nie, aangesien daar 'n groot blootstelling aan onbekende stimuli binne die klaskamersituasie is. 'n aspek wat deurgaans aandagsvermoens nadelig be'invloed het, was wanneer die sein 10 db swakker as die maskering was. dit is 'n belangrike waarneming met betrekking tot voorligting aan die onderwyser(es) van die jonger groep in die1, skool (gr i). daar sal beslis deurgaans probleme ondervind word deur die kinders om aandag te skenk aan 'n stimulus te midde van omgewingslawaai en dit moet in gedagte gehou word tydens onderrig of aanbieding van veral onbekende of nuwe materiaal. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditiewe aandag 33 hierdie gegewens is ook belangrik vir die oudioloog aangesien dit die response op sein-tot-ruistoetse definitief sal be'invloed. response op so 'n toets kan dus nie as betroubaar beskou word nie. opsommend kan dus gese word dat die ontwikkelingsproses van ouditiewe aandag by die groep 5;2 tot 5;8 voldoende is om aan die eise wat binne die formele onderrigsituasie gestel word, te voldoen. wanneer daar dus 'n leerprobleem opgemerk word, sal dit ook belangrik wees om ondersoek in te stel na die ontwikkeling van ouditiewe aandagsvermoens. ouditiewe aandagsprosessering verdien 'n regmatige plek in navorsingstudies en behoort ook as 'n belangrike aspek gereken te word wanneer die jong kind 'n probleem, byvoorbeeld 'n vertraagde taalontwikkeling, presenteer: in childhood the delay in language acquisition, poverty, or inarticulate oral expression, difficulty in responding to oral instructions, apparent difficulty in appreciating the sequence of oral instruction, and difficulty in sound localization would suggest central dysfunction. if these manifestations represent an absence of function when they should be present, a developmental origin should be suspected and when there is a decline in these functions which are described to have been previously present, acquired central nervous system disorder should be suspected.4 as ouditiewe aandag beskou word as 'n integrale deel van ouditiewe persepsie is dit ook dus 'n sentrale proses en kan kennis daaromtrent van groot diagnostiese waarde wees. dankbetuigings mev s r hugo en mnr η ε c tesner, departement spraakwetenskap, spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria, vir hulle hulp gedurende die studie. prof d j stoker, hoof van die departement statistiek, universiteit van pretoria, vir die statistiese ontledings van die resultate. mnr ρ meffert, departement akoestiek, wnnr, vir die tegniese samestelling van die toetsmateriaal. verwysings 1. botha, d. (red) (76/77): summarized outlines on the pharmacology of important drug groups. mims desk reference, 12, 26-31. 2. caplan, l. r. (1978): variability of perceptual functioning: the sensory cortex as a "categorizer" and "deducer". brain and language, 6, 1 1 3 . 3. carhart, r. (1967): binaural reception of meaningful material. in: a. b. graham sensorineural hearing processes and disorders. little, brown and company, massachusetts. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) elmarie niemand 34 4 duane, d. d. 1977: central auditory dysfunction in: keith, r. a neurologic perspective of central auditory dysfunction. grune & stratton n.y. 5 fox, j· (1974): recognition and attention. quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 26, 144-157. 6. guyton, a. c. (1966): function of the human body. w. b. saunders company, philadelphia. 7. johnson, d. j. & myklebust, h. r. (1967): learning disabilities: educational principles and practices. grune and stratton, n.y. 8. johnson, p. j. et al (1974): effects of category attention, relative frequency of relevant values and practice on attribute identification performance. journal of experimental psychology, 103, 160-166. 9. jordaan, w. j. et al (1975): algemene sielkunde: 'n psigobiologiese benadering. mcgraw-hill, johannesburg. 10. luria, a . r. (1973): the working brain: an introduction to neuropsychology. the penguin press, allen lane. 11. martin, m. (1978): retention of attended and unattended auditorily and visually presented material. quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 30, 187-200. 12. meldman, m. j. (1970): diseases of attention and perception. pergamon press, n.y. 13. moray, n. (1969): attention: selective processes in vision and hearing. hutchinson educational, london. 14. moray, n. et al (1976): attention to pure tone. quarterly journal of experimental psychology, 28, 271-283. 15. myers, t. f. (1970): asymmetry and attention in phonic decoding. in: sanders, a. f. (red). attenion and performance iii: proceedings of a symposium on attention and performance. north-holland publishing company, amsterdam. 16. neisser, u . (1967): cognitive psychology. meridith publishing company, n.y. 17. northern, j. l. & downs, m. p. (1974): hearing in children. the williams and wilkins company, maryland. 18. rydberg, s. & arnberg, p. w. (1976): attending and processing broadened within children's concept learning. journal of experimental child psychology, 22, 161-177. 19. saunders, d. a. (1977): auditory perception of speech: an introduction to principles and problems. prentice hall, n-y20. shiffrin, r. m. et al (1976): attending to fourty-nine spatial positions at once. journal of experimental psychology: human perception and performance, 2, 14-22. 21. simon, h . a . (1972): on the development of the processor: in: farnham-diggory, s. (red). information processing in children. academic press, n.y. j 22. underwood, g. (1977): attention, awareness and hemispheric differences in word recognition. neuropsychologia, 15, 61-67. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouditiewe aandag 35 23. walley, r. e. & weiden, t. d. (1973): lateral inhibition and cognitive masking: a neuropsychological theory of attention. psychological review, 80, 284-302. 24. yusson, s. r. & levy, v. m. (jnr.) (1975): effects of warm and neutral models on the attention of observational learners. journal of experimental child psychology, 20, 66-72. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aids for • the development of perception • the acquisition of speech and language skills • the improvement of motor co-ordination plus • helpful texts for therapists • educational toys, books and equipment • records for auditory training • catalogues on request • large variety of tests available stockists of • learning to listen • two sound lottos • "listen what is that?" play and schoolroom 8 tyrwhitt avenue, rosebank (adjoining the constantia cinema) telephones: 788-1304 i p.o. box 52137, saxonwold, 2132 ο m the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 21 the development of a framework for assessing developing conversational skills susan rumble, b.sc. (log) (cape town) hearing and speech clinic, tygerberg hospital karen malan, m.a. (appl. linguistics) (reading, u.k.) department of logopaedics, university of cape town abstract a framework for investigating the development of conversational skills in children, comprising the areas of topic control, repair ofcomlinguistic cohesion, was devised. this was undertaken bya process f . ^ ^ ^ ^ t j s i oraamatic profiles and the developmental literature. the framework was then modified inductively to accommodate features of the data ^iha^ed&om 12 normally developing english-speaking children in the aye groups three, four and five years. a number of aye-related trends assessment procedures which will aid the collection of normative data presently lacking. opsomming vi raamwerk om die ontwikkeling van gesprekvaardighede, bestaande uit instandhouding en gespreksonderwerp, herstel van komnozaat^ss^sspr^dekindcrs, in die ouderdomsgroepe drie, vier en vyfjaar te akkommodeer verskeie ontwikkehngss olelstem met bevindinge in die ontwikkelingsliteratuur. hierdie studie beklemtoon lebehoefte aan meer verfynde evaluasieprosedures wat sodoende die versameling van normatiewe data, wat tans ontoereikend is, sal bevorder. the development of pragmatic language skills has been perhaps the most rapidly expanding area of child language research over the last decade. much of the work in this area has focused on either communicative intentions (the functions -that speech acts serve for the speaker) or the skills involved in conversational competence (including presupposition and the social organization of discourse). however, while the acquisition of communicative intentions has been quite extensively researched (klecan-aker & swank, 1988), the available information on developing conversational skills, particularly in the pre-school population, is as yet fragmented and sparse. the absence of adequate baseline data on conversational development in normal communicators has hampered attempts to construct clinically applicable frameworks for assessing the conversational skills of language disordered children. compounding the difficulty are disagreements over definitions of conversation and the scope of conversational analysis, with a consequent lack of coherent models for studying its development and for developing assessment instruments. of the commonly available clinical protocols (eg. prutting & kirchner, 1987; penn, 1985; roth & spekman, 1984; wollner & geller, 1982), most comprise categories designed to provide a measure of overall communicative ability and hence lack the specificity required for in-depth analysis of conversation. further, some were developed explicitly for use with older age groups penn's (1985j profile of communicative appropriateness for adult populations and prutting & kirchner's (1987) pragmatic die suid-afrikaans tydskrif vir kommunikasiawykings, vol. 37 1990 protocol for subjects older than 5 years. wollner & geller's (1982) communication profile and roth & spekman's (1984) organizational framework for assessing pragmatic skills, although derived from the developmental literature, are plagued by problems of significant gaps in developmental information and have been criticized for their lack of discrete, well-motivated categories of description (mctear & contiramsden, 1989). a need exists, therefore, for assessment frameworks that focus specifically on conversational competence and that are sensi: tive to developmental trends in skills in this area in pre-school children. paired with the need for detailed normative data is the requirement for adequate models of the skills involved in conversation. mctear (1985) observes that models of conversational competence should account for increasing sophistication in three basic areas: (i) topic control (involving turn-taking and the structuring of conversational turns), (ii) repair of conversational breakdown· and (hi) the use of linguistic cohesive devices and presuppositions. he argues that in addition to the acquisition of detailed normative data, more attention should be paid to the validity of descriptive analyses and to providing reliable criteria for the recognition of analytic categories. bearing these requirements in mind, the present study was designed as a preliminary attempt to develop a framework for evaluating conversational skills that would reflect developmental trends in the pre-school age group. " sasha 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 susan rumble and karen malan methodology aims 1. the development of a framework for the assessment of developing conversational skills in the areas of topic control, repair of communication breakdown and linguistic cohesion. 2. use of the assessment framework to investigate developmental trends in conversational skills in normally-developing children aged 3 to 5 years. procedure procedures for fulfilling the two aims overlapped, in that the assessment framework was further modified to accommodate specific features of the data obtained from normally developing children, in order to ensure its sensitivity to the developmental changes that emerged. stage 1: a preliminary assessment framework was devised, by combining selected portions of two existing pragmatic profiles. penn's (1985) profile of communicative appropriateness (pca), although developed for use with adult clinical populations, includes three sections (response to interlocutor, control of semantic content and cohesion) which fall within the scope of conversational analysis defined here; these were therefore included in the preliminary framework. since the pca does not specifically examine repair of conversational breakdown, section iii ofwollner & geller's (1982) communication profile, (communication breakdown, dealing with repair strategies), was added. section ii (conversational acts, dealing with ability to initiate, extend and terminate topics) was also included, with the intention of comparing its usefulness with the corresponding section of the pca (control of semantic content). stage 2: samples of conversation were collected from 12 normally-developing, english-speaking children in three agegroups (see table 1) attending a creche for coloured children of groote schuur hospital employees. both sexes were equally represented. audiometric and language screening measures were undertaken to ensure normal hearing and language development in all subjects. table is subject characteristics age-group no. of s's mean age age range 3 years 4 3.6 years 3.4 3.9 4 years 4 4.5 years 4.3 4.7 5 years 4 5.3 years 5.1 5.5 each subject was audioand video-taped while conversing individually with the investigator in a familiar room at the creche. following the suggestion of brinton et al. (1986) and mctear (1985), a naturalistic setting was used. tasks used were those which had been established in a prior pilot study (outlined in rumble, 1988) to be most successful in generating spontaneous conversation: doll play; free conversation on topics initiated by the investigator relevant to the subjects' environment and a story re-tell task, following the suggestion of griffiths et al. (1986) and liles (1987) who advocate narrative tasks to investigate use of linguistic cohesion devices. roth & spekman's (1984) strategies for creating communication breakdown (mumbling intentionally, responding with a noncontingent reply, providing inadequate or ambiguous instructions for performing a task) were utilized, a consistent number of times in each subject interaction, at regular points during f the conversation. data from each subject was orthographically transcribed from the video tapes, including contextual, non-verbal and prosodic information. stage 3: the preliminary assessment framework was modified by coding the data obtained from one subject in each age group according to the categories of the preliminary framework. modifications were necessitated by difficulties related to the use of different scorin'g and coding systems for sections of the framework obtained from different sources. furthermore, expansion, re-ordering and omission of certain categories was necessary to reflect subtle developmental trends. information from the developmental literature was consulted for this purpose and included in the final framework. thus, an inductive process was employed whereby descriptive categories evolved out of information reported in the literature and were subsequently modified to accommodate features that emerged from the data. the information was organized into a single framework comprising the superordinate categories of topic control, repair of communication breakdown and linguistic cohesion. stage 4: data from all 12 subjects was coded and analyzed to the final assessment framework. all utterances were coded for the linguistic cohesion and repair of breakdown sections, whereas only those produced during the free conversation task were coded for the topic control section. for the latter section, the investigator's utterances were also coded. coding of data was undertaken independently by two trained speech and language pathologists from observation of the videotapes. both raters were familiar with the defining criteria for recognition of categories. point to point inter-rater reliability was calculated at 80%, above silverman's (1977) criterion of 75%. frequencies of occurrence of each category were calculated for eaeh-subject. these frequency scores were then subjected to one-way analysis of variance tests to determine if differences between these frequencies were statistically significant. | results and discussion 1 the categories of conversational skills that emerged in the process of developing a final assessment framework are shown in tables 2 , 3 and 4. discussion of the specific modifications and expansions of categories undertaken in developing and refining the original descriptive categories to accommodate the developmental data is not within the scope of this paper. a description of these processes, together with full definitions and examples of all categories in the final framework is provided in rumble (1988). the discussion here will focus on the developmental data gathered from the normally developing subjects. tables 2,3 and 4 also provide, for each age group, the mean frequencies of occurrence of the categories of conversational skills in each section of the framework. few o f t h e comparisons of frequencies across age groups yielded statistically significant results, presumably due, at least in part, to the small sample size which magnifies the-effects of individual variation on overall comparisons. despite this, a number of developmental trends were evident; these are discussed below for each section of the framework. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a framework for assessing developing conversational skills 2 3 topic control 1 initiatory acts (ia): table 2 shows that 3 and 4 year olds initiated a greater number of topics overall than did the 5 year olds, a trend also reported by wanska & bedrosian (1985). however, at least half the ias produced by all subjects were inappropriate, as they occurred where responses to previous turns were expected. both prutting (1979) and mctear (1985) note that children from 2 to 3 years exhibit problems maintaining and extending the topic of conversation, resulting in inappropriate and rapid changes of topic throughout their discourse. the drop in number of topic initiatory acts after 4 years may thus reflect an increasing ability to use more appropriate strategies for topic maintenance, with a corresponding decreasing dependency on initiatory acts to serve this function. table 2: mean frequency scores per age group for the categories of topic control category initiatory act investigator subjects topic relevant response investigator subjects: verbal acknowledgement verbal response to interrogative nonverbal acknowledgement nonverbal response ; to interrogative topic relevant act investigator subjects off topic act inappropriate response no response age group 3 years 4 years 5 years 5.0 4.5 32.75 0.5 15.25 3.5 19.75 37.5 9.0' 2.5 6.5 6.5 4.5 38.0 1.0 19.0 3.5 8.5 33.5 26.75* 1.0 4.25 6.25 2.5 30.0 0.25 22.25 1.5 6.5 32.75 14.75* 4.0 3.5 * 4 year group produced significantly more than 3 year group (p=0.05) 2. topic relevant responses (trr): these occur in response to an utterance of the previous speaker. table 2 shows that for subjects in all three age groups, a far greater proportion (90%) of trrs were responses to interrogatives, rather than acknowledgements of previous turns, a finding supported by bloom et al. (1976) and mctear (1985). the tendency for adults to rely heavily on interrogatives in dialogue with young children is well documented (corsaro, 1979) and may serve to facilitate conversational development by providing the child with opportunities to provide linguistically contingent replies, thus keeping the conversational ball in play. table 2 also shows that subjects' use of verbal trrs increased from 3 to 5 years, while the frequency of non-verbal trrs declined, reflecting an expanding linguistic competence and a corresponding diminishing reliance on non-linguistic modes of response to maintain discourse topics. 3. topic relevant acts (tra): these are spontaneous utterances that go beyond the previous turn by adding new information of relevance to the topic. table 2 shows a higher occurrence of tras in the 4 and 5 year old groups, with the increase from 3 to 4 years being statistically significant. many more tras were produced by the investigator in all age groups, a trend also noted by corsaro (1979) who observed that adults generally respond to children's utterances with tras, while children tend to respond to adults' tras with trrs. the number of investigator-tras diminished with oldel subjects who provided greater numbers of tras themselves. 4. off topic acts either inappropriate responses to questions or failure to respond where a response was expected showed a general tendency to diminish with age, corresponding to the increase in appropriate verbal responses to interrogatives. 5. mean number of utterances per turn: in interactions with all age groups, the investigator produced a greater number of utterances per turn than subjects. the data on topic control generally confirmed mctear's (1985) observation that from 3 to 5 years the major development is from relatively closed exchanges with an initiaterespond structure to more extended sequences of dialogue. by 5 years of age the children here showed increased ability to maintain and extend a topic of conversation in the form of topic relevant acts and responses. yet, even at this age, the adult remained the dominant member of the communicative dyad, taking responsibility for topic control in the form of a high number of interrogatives and extensions of the child's utterances (tras) to ensure shared meaning, as well as producing an overall greater quantity of utterances at each turn. repair of conversational breakdown 1. clarification requests: some clear patterns emerged here with regard to a sequence of development. as table 3 shows, requests for clarification were just beginning to emerge in the 3 year old group, supporting both gallagher (1977) and garvey's (1979) observation that children do not produce contingent queries until 3 years of age. clarification requests were produced incrementally by one 3 year old, two 4 year olds and all five year olds. first to emerge, at 3 years, were specific requests for confirmation, where the child requests confirmation of some element of the previous utterance about which he is uncertain, as in the following example. i : 'pass me another chair s: tets (s holds up chair) i : yes non-specific requests for repetition emerged at 4 years; like specific requests for confirmation, these do not specify which part of the previous utterance needs to be clarified: i : 'give me another one s: hmm i : 'pass me another chair die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif ir kommuniksieafikinis, vol. 37. 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 2 4 table 3: mean frequency scores per age group for the categories of repair of conversational breakdown age group category 3 years 4 years 5 years clarification requests non-specific request for repetition 0. 0.25 1.25 specific request for repetition 0. 0. 0.25 specific request for confirmation 0.5 0.5 1.0 specific request for specification 0. 0.75 1.25 repair strategies revision 0. 0.5 0. addition 4.25** 1.75** 1.5** cue 0. 0. 0. whole repetition 2.25 2.5 1.25 part repetition 0. 1.0 1.25 no response 1.75 1.0 1.25 * * 3 year group produced s 5 year group (p=0.01) ignificantly more than 4 and more demanding linguistically are requests that require identification of specific ambiguous or missing information, as in specific requests for specification and specific requests for repetition. the former emerged at 4 years and is illustrated in the following example. 1 : those 'people who came to your creche that were 'telling you stories s : what people the latter was produced only once, by a 5 year old subject: 1 : put the 'cat in the box s : the (pause 2 seconds) (s looks questioningly at i) * i : cat 2. repair strategies: as is evident from table 3, the developmental picture with regard to use of repair strategies in response to requests for clarification was less clear, as subjects showed a fairly high degree of variability and inconsistency in their use of these strategies. nevertheless, certain strong tendencies emerged. 3 year olds produced a significantly greater number of addition repairs (involving addition of information not provided in the original utterance) than 4 or 5 year olds. the relative linguistic immaturity of this age group may account for this : 3 susan rumble and karen malan year olds frequently produced utterances containing either insufficient information or inappropriate anaphoric and demonstrative reference (discussed below), necessitating a large number of clarification requests for additional information, as the following example shows. i : 'what was the mommy saying here s : birthday i wlibse birthday s : the girl's birthday whilst the frequency of addition repairs declined in the older age groups, 4 and 5 year olds most frequently used repetition repairs, involving repetition of the whole or part of a previous utterance. the trend here, shown in table 3, was for a decrease in whole repetitions and an increase in part repetitions with increasing age, reflecting a growing ability to distinguish which specific linguistic elements require repeating. adults may play a facilitative role in this process: the majority of part repetitions in the data were produced in response to specific requests for specification which assist the child in determining which elements need clarification. revision repair is a more complex linguistic strategy than repetition or addition, involving recoding of the message in the form of alterations to syntactic structure; these were used by only one child, a 4 year old. no examples of cues (involving definition of terms from the original utterance or providing relevant background information for its interpretation) occurred, suggesting that these require a level of metalinguistic abstraction that is beyond the 5 year old level. to summarize, the findings suggest that the development of effective strategies for dealing with conversational breakdown depends at least in part on the ability to identify specific information bearing elements of a message. this ability did not begin to manifest in either clarification requests or repair strategies before 4years of age, although repair strategies were certainly in evidence in younger children of 3 years. further, it seems the ability to recode surface syntactic or lexical aspects of the message in formulating repairs is a skill developed only from 5 years and beyond. | linguistic cohesion ; ι 1. anaphoric reference: this category comprises pronouns that refer to previously identified referents. they were coded as appropriate when interpretable either with the aid of contextual cues or due to the referent having been previously identified. table 4 shows that many more instances of anaphoric reference used by subjects were appropriate than inappropriate and that appropriate use increased with age. however, many of the appropriate instances may have been considered inappropriate in the absence of contextual cues to aid the investigator's interpretation. appropriate use of pronouns is closely linked to the ability to make correct assumptions regarding given versus new infor: mation. bates & macwhinney (1979) point out that inappropriate use of pronouns (ie. without prior identification of the referent) does not necessarily imply lack'of awareness that pronouns specify given information, but rather that the child makes incorrect assumptions about the 'givenness' of information. in the data reported here, such incorrect assumptions were found in all age groups. further, the use of rising intonathe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37,1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of a framework for assessing developing conversational skills 2 5 table 4: mean frequency scores per age group for the categories of linguistic cohesion category age group 3 years 4 years 5 years anaphoric reference appropriate inappropriate 16.25 5.5 27.5 21.25 28.75 9.25 demonstrative reference appropriate inappropriate 22.5 2.0 18.5 2.0 23.25 1.0 substitution appropriate inappropriate 6.25 11.75 6.5 2.75 8.25 0.25 ellipsis appropriate inappropriate 15.25* 6.75 23.0* 3.0 27.5* 3.75 additive conjunction 7.25 15.75 6.0 causal conjunction 1.0 3.0 1.25 temporal conjunction 1.0 4.75 6.5 antithesis conjunction 0.25 0.25 1.75 * 5 year group produced significantly more than 3 year group (p=0.05) tion accompanying information that the child assumed was shared was a feature used consistently by all subjects. this is illustrated in the following example from a 5 year old who incorrectly assumed that the investigator knew who the members of her household were. i : so 'who lives in your, house then s : v ^ i : who's we s : 0 (s looks puzzled) • 2. demonstrative reference: this refers to the use of deictic terms such as 'this', 'that', 'here', 'there', which refer to referents by specifying their location on the dimension of distance. table 4 indicates that subjects in all age groups had attained the ability to encode these deictic relations linguistically. the vast majority of instances of demonstrative reference were apropriate, with inappropriate use (where the referent was not clear from the context) decreasing slightly at 5 years. these findings are consistent with the observations of both bloom et al. (1976) and bates & macwhinney (1979) that children as young as 3 years show awareness of deictic shift by correct use of demonstrative pronouns. 3. ellipsis: use of elliptical utterances allows the speaker to reduce redundancy in a message by encoding only the essential elements; it therefore relies on the ability to identify given information in prior messages which can then be deleted in the elliptical utterance. table 4 shows that appropriate ellipsis was present from 3 years, its use increasing significantly with age while inappropriate use declined. these trends were closely associated with type of ellipsis. clausal ellipsis (involving ellision of both noun and verb phrase) results in encoding of minimal information; this occurred with greater frequency in all age groups than either nominal or verbal ellipsis. a large amount of inappropriate clausal ellipsis occurred in the data from 3 year olds, who were unaware that further specification was necessary. the use of appropriate nominal and verbal ellipsis increased with age, often prompted by specific requests for specification from the investigator. their use appears to coincide with the development of increasing ability to take the listener's needs into account and consequently to select the appropriate elements of a message to encode. 4. substitution: this category refers to items other than personal pronouns which replace previously identified elements. as is evident from table 4, the data here suggests that correct use of substitution is possibly a later acquired form of alternative coding then ellipsis. only two 3 year olds used substitution appropriately whereas all of them used appropriate ellipsis and far fewer appropriate instances of substitution than ellipsis occurred overall for the whole subject group. all instances of substitution in the data were nominal (one word substituting for a noun phrase), except for one example of verbal substitution (one word substituting for a verb phrase), produced by a 5 year old. thus verbal and clausal substitution may be more complex forms acquired only after 5 years. 5. conjunction: conjunctions serve a cohesive function in discourse when they relate successive utterances to each other across speaking turns. ervin-tripp (1978) described a tentative sequence of development of conjunction use across turns: additive (and, too, also) followed by temporal (then), causal (so, because), and then antithesis (but). the frequencies shown in table 4 for conjunction use replicate this sequence exactly. additive conjunctions were used more frequently than any other type by all age groups; this is in accordance with mctear's (1985) observation that a 5 year old's 'and' is still the main cohesive conjunction used. an interesting feature of these subjects' conjunction usage which is not reported elsewhere in the literature, however, was the use of 'now' (as well as 'and') to express additive relations and 'so' to express temporal relations, as shown in the following examples. i : that's the shower s : now 'where's the tap (additive) i : now 'what happens next s : so the 'pram ride by itself (temporal) these features are assumed to be a feature of the dialect of english spoken by the subjects,since they were used by all the children in this study. to summarize, the data on cohesion indicated that correct use of all types of cohesive devices investigated was present to some degree from 3 years of age, with a general trend for die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif rir kommunikasieafiviikiniis, vol. 37. 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 susan rumble and karen malan increasing apropriate use and a decrease in inappropriate use up to 5 years. developing competence in the use of cohesive devices seemingly involved the ability to scan prior messages, make correct assumptions regarding the need for new as opposed to given information and to take the listener's perspective into account. conclusion this study has emphasized the need for the development of more refined procedures for assessing conversational skills in children, as well as for the collection of normative data. the procedure employed here, of collating descriptive categories from previous research and modifying these inductively to accommodate features of the data collected from normally developing children, proved useful in ensuring that the resultant categories were adequately motivated in terms of their relevance to developing language. further, the use of small subject samples in successive age levels, while raising problems of generalizability, allowed for detailed descriptive analysis of relatively subtle developmental trends. given the paucity of present knowledge about the development of conversation, this approach may be preferably to a more superficial analysis of larger samples. it would certainly be premature at this stage to consider the framework provided here as a clinically applicable tool. mctear (1985) rightly cautions that we are a long way off from being able to produce profiles of conversational development on analogy with similar existing profiles for areas such as syntax and phonology. we would suggest that a great deal more groundwork is required in the form of careful refinement of descriptive categories on the basis of developmental information. additionally, several problematical theoretical and methodological issues require attention if the goal of an effective clinical tool is to be realized. for one thing, findings from naturalistic methods of data collection, such as that used in this study, are invariably limited by the fact that conclusions are drawn regarding subjects' linguistic competence on the basis of their performance in a limited and particular sample of interaction. thus, further research efforts to validate the descriptive categories derived here should include systematic exploration of the effects on performance of contextual factors such as variations in communicative partner and physical setting. it seems likely, for instance, that important differences may exist between adult-child and child-child discourse. future research will also need to address the issue of how best to 'score' categories of conversational behaviour in clinical assessment; as mctear & conti-ramsden(1989) point out, both frequency counts and judgements of appropriacy are problematical as indices of conversational analysis. finally, it is possible that descriptive categories based on normal development may not be sensitive to specific problems that arise in language disorder, suggesting the need for testing the assessment framework with language impaired children. references bates, e. & macwhinney, b.a. a functionalist approach to the acquisition of grammar. in e. ochs and b.b. schieffelin (eds.) developmental pragmatics. new york: academic press, 1979. bloom, l., rocissano, l. & hood, l. adult-child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. cognitive psychology, 8, 521-522, 1976. brinton, b., fujiki, m., winkler, e. & loeb, d.f. development of conversational repair strategies in response to request for clarification./ speech hear. res., 29, 75-81, 1986. corsaro, w.a. sociolinguistic patterns in adult-child discourse. in e. ochs & b.b. schieffelin (eds) developmental pragmatics. new york: academic press, 1979. ervin-tripp, s. some features of early child-adult dialogues. language in society, 7, 357-73, 1978. gallagher, t.m. revision behaviours in the speech of normal children developing language./ speech hear. res., 20, 303-318, 1977. garvey, c. contingent queries and their relations in discourse. in e. ochs & b.b. schieffelin (eds) developmental pragmatics. new york: academic press, 1979. griffith, p.i., ripich, p.n. & dastoli, s.l. story structure, cohesion and propositions in story recalls by learning disabled and nondisabled children. journal of psycholinguistic research, 15(6), 539-555, 1986. klecan-aker, j.s. & swank, p.r. the use of a pragmatic protocol with normal preschool children./ comm. dis., 21, 85-102, 1988. liles, b.z. episode organization and cohesive conjunctives in narratives of children with and without language disorder./ speech hear. res., 30(2), 185-196, 1987. mctear, m.f. children's conversation. oxford: basil blackwell 1985. mctear, m.f. & conti-ramsden, g. assessment of pragmatics. in k. grundy (ed.) linguistics in clinical practice. taylor and francis, 1989. penn, c. the profile of communicative appropriateness: a clinical tool for the assessment of pragmatics. south african journal of communication disorders, 32, 18-25, 1985. prutting, c.a. process: the action of moving forward progressively from one point to another on the way to completion./ speech hear. dis., 44(1), 3-30, 1979. prutting, c.a. & kirchner, d.a. a clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language./ speech hear. dis., 52, 105-119, 1987. roth, f.p. & spekman, n.j. assessing the pragmatic abilities of children. part i: organization framework and assessment parameters./ speech hear. dis., 49, 2-11, 1984. rumble, s. the development of a framework for assessing children's conversational skills and its use in a preliminary investigation into developmental trends in normal three, four and five year olds. unpublished research report, department of logopaedics; university of cape town, 1988. silverman, s. research design in speech pathology and audiology. new, jersey: prentice-hall, 1977. / j wanska, s.k. & bedrosian, j.l. conversational structure and topic) performance in mother-child interaction./ speech hear res 28,579-584,1985. '' wollner, s. & geller, e. methods of assessing pragmatic abilities. in 1 j.v. irwin (ed) pragmatics: the role in language development.! university of laverne: fox point publishing, 1982. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 37, 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationship between phonology and inflectional morphology in an agrammatic aphasic meryl kobrin ba (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) lesley wolk ma (speech pathology) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract the interaction between phonological and morphological breakdown in an agrammatic aphasic was investigated. three linguistic tasks were constructed which were presented via two modes, reading and repetition. results revealed that purely phonological consonant clusters were easier than clusters which contain a morphological component, and that these categories could be differentiated in terms of phonological error type. inflectional omission was conditioned by phonological characteristics of the preceding segment. there was an interaction between the phonological and morphological hierarchies of difficulty in inflections which are homonyms phonologically. findings suggest an interdependence between phonological and morphological breakdown in the agrammatic aphasic examined. results were discussed with reference to clinical implications. opsomming die interaksie tussen fonologiese en morfologiese uitvalle in 'n agrammatiese afasia pasient is ondersoek. drie linguistiese take is opgestel. die pasient moes die take ouditief (deur middel van herhaling) en visueel (deur middel van lees) uitvoer. resultate dui daarop dat suiwer fonologiese konsonant groepe makliker was om uit te voer, as groepe wat 'n morfologiese komponent bevat het en dat hierdie kategoriee gedifferensieer kon word in terme van tipe fonologiese foute. die voorafgaande segment se fonologiese karakteristieke het inflektiewe weglatings bepaal. daar was interaksie tussen die fonologiese en morfologiese hierargiese moeilikheidswaarde van infleksies wat fonologiese homonieme is. bevindings dui op 'n interafhanklikheid tussen fonologiese en morfologiese uitvalle in die pasient. resultate is bespreek met verwysing na kliniese implikasies. to date, the trend within the psycholinguistic aphasia research has been to focus on the components of language (syntax, semantics and phonology) in isolation, rather than to investigate interrelationships between these levels of linguistic breakdown. the symptomatology of agrammatic aphasics, particularly their tendency to delete inflectional morphemes and their high proportion of phonemic errors, provides a unique opportunity to examine the mutual influence of phonologically i and morphologically impaired systems. independent research into phonology and inflectional morphology has been well documented. articulatory investigations have resulted in conflicting opinions as regards the nature of aphasic error performance. johns and darley (19j70) and shankweiler and harris (1973), for example, support the notion that phonemic substitutions are primarily random, variable and unrelated to the target sound. other investigators suggest that aphasic articulatory errors reflect systematic, rule-governed variations from the target phonemes (blumstein, 1973; marquardt, reinhart and peterson, 1979). studies exploring the performance of agrammatic aphasics on inflectional endings reflect a consistent hierarchy of difficulty for the various morphemes (goodglass and berko, 1960; goodglass, 1976). de villiers (1974) contends that explanations such as transformational complexity, semantic complexity, stress, redundancy and frequency of occurrence of each morpheme in normal adult speech are insufficient to explain the hierarchical morphemic impairment. this suggests that alternative explanations should be sought. a number of theories have been proposed to account for the underlying deficits in agrammatism. kean (1977) contends that agrammatism is an " . . . interaction between an impaired phonological capacity and otherwise intact linguistic capacities" (p. 10). this condie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 troversial phonological explanation has subsequently been criticized. garman (1981) suggests that a number of kean's arguments are based on misinterpretations of the existing literature. kolk (1978) argues that although a phonological approach may have value with respect to the 'articulation' impairment in agrammatism, it does not provide a convincing argument to explain the syntactic omissions characteristic of these patients. goodglass and berko (1960) take an opposing view to kean (1977) and suggest that grammatical function is more important than phonological structure in determining the difficulty of an inflectional ending. this theory is based on their finding that the plural, possessive and third person singular inflectional morphemes (all of which are homonyms phonologically) are omitted with differential frequency in agrammatic aphasics (goodglass and berko, 1960). martin, wasserman, gilden, gerstman and west (1975) suggest that neither a purely phonological nor a purely morphological breakdown is sufficient to explain aphasic error performance. they propose that " . . . it is the interaction of processes which is affected in aphasia rather than a specific impairment of a particular process or component" (p. 449). this interactional model between phonological and morphological impairment has not been confirmed in the aphasia literature. however, several studies in child language have shown an interaction between syntax and phonology (menyuk and looney, 1972; paul and shriberg, 1982). the paucity of research into the relationship between linguistic components in aphasia, provided a strong motivation for this study. the broad goal was thus to investigate the inter-relationship between phonological and morphological impairment in the expressive language of an agrammatic aphasic. the specific aims were: 1. to compare the subject's error performance on consonant clusters which are purely phonological constructions (pc); clusters which © sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 meryl kobrin and lesley wolk are phonological constructions but with morphological poss talities (pcm); and clusters which are morphological combinations (mc) 2 to establish whether the subject's omission of inflectional morphemes is conditioned by the sonorance hierarchy of the preceding segment, as suggested by kean (1977). 3. to examine the subject's production of three grammatical morphemes which are homonyms phonologically, namely the plural marker, the possessive marker and the third person singular, all of which are realized morphophonemically by the allophones /s,z,az/. method subject the subject used in this study was r.p., a white, south african, english speaking female, aged thirty-eight years. in december 1978, she presented with a sudden onset of expressive aphasia. computerized tomography revealed a left middle cerebral artery infarct, the etiology of which was unknown. no further neurological details were available. pre-morbidly, she was right handed. r.p. fulfilled the following criteria: 1. she was a moderately impaired agrammatic aphasic as assessed on the boston diagnostic aphasia examination (bdae) (goodglass and kaplan, 1972). 2. r.p. demonstrated phonemic errors, particularly on consonant clusters. 3. her expressive language was characterized by omission of inflectional morphemes. 4. dysarthria and oro-facial apraxia were excluded as being causally related to the phonemic errors. 5. phonemic discrimination abilities were excluded as being etiologically related to phonemic errors. 6. r.p. demonstrated a competence for the tasks on which she would be expected to perform. more specifically, reading and auditory comprehension abilities, as assessed on the bdae were sufficiently intact to enable these modalities to be utilized in testing. 7. r.p.'s mother tongue was english. 8. peripheral hearing and vision were within normal limits. 9. r.p. was neurologically stable during the test period. tasks and procedure a. preliminary investigations on the bdae, r.p. obtained a profile representing broca's (agrammatic) aphasia. results served to satisfy some of the criteria for subject selection, specifically her relatively intact receptive language and reading abilities and the presence of phonemic and morphological errors. on the goldman fristoe test of articulation (goldman and fristoe, 1969) r.p. showed several articulation errors on both single phonemes and phonemic sequences, verifying the presence of phonemic errors in meaningful words as elicited on a naming task. on a test of ten english inflectional morphemes, designed by the authors, r.p. demonstrated inflectional omission. in accordance with the format proposed by goodglass and berko (1960) a sentence completion test was constructed to assess the following morphenes: plural /s,z/; plural /sz/; past /t,d/; past /ad/; present singular /s,z/; present singular /sz/; possessive /s,z/; possessive /sz/; comparative /a/; superlative /sst/. the test included six opportunities for the use of each morpheme selected. the following is an example of an item (plural) "i bought a large pot, a medium-sized pot and a small pot. altogether i bought three ? " . on the goldman fristoe test of auditory discrimination (goldman, fristoe and woodcock, 1970), administered in order to verify the subject's competence for discriminating between single consonants, r.p. scored 100%, indicating no errors on this standardized test of auditory discrimination. r.p. responded adequately at all frequencies on a screening pure tone audiometric test, indicating that hearing was within normal limits. b. tasks all tasks designed for the purpose of this study were evaluated by means of a pilot study on three normal adults. 1. ccvcc word list a list of 150 ccvcc words (appendix i) was devised in accordance with the format proposed by martin et al,. (1975). the stimuli were divided into three groups of fifty words each. in the first group, the final cluster was a purely phonological construction (pc) such as /mp/ in 'cramp'. in the second group, the final consonant cluster was a phonological construction, but the final segment belonged to the group /s,z,t,d/ and therefore suggested the possibility of a morpheme (martin et al., 1975). an example of a phonological construction with the morphological possibilities (pcm) is /st/ in 'breast'. the third group contained final consonant clusters which were morphological combinations (mc), such as /st/ in 'dressed'. the inflections included in the (mc) list in the present study were limited to the plural /s,z/ and past /t,d/, and in order to maintain uniformity, words in the pcm group were limited to the phonemes (s,z,t,d/ in final consonant position. 2. sonorance-inflection word list a list of 150 words was composed (appendix ii). each word was a combination of a stem morpheme (cv or cvc) and an inflectional morpheme (past /d/ or plural (z(), for example (bees, called). the stem morphemes were divided into five groups of thirty words each, according to the sonorance hierarchy of the final segment of the stem. sonorance was used to refer to the extent to which the airflow is impeded during the articulation of a segment (kean, 1977). the five categories of final stem segments arranged hierarchically from the most sonorant (least impeded airflow) to the least sonorant (most impeded airflow) were: vowels and diphthongs, liquids, nasals, fricatives and stops respectively. within each group fifteen words were combined with the plural inflectional allophone /z/ and fifteen words with the past inflectional allophone /d/. i rationale for selecting the allophones /z/ and /d/ i i since stems ending in a vowel are constrained by morphophonemic rules to take a voice allophone, voiced allophones were used throughout. several studies in the aphasic literature have shown that' the plural is a relatively well retained morpheme whereas the past regular is a frequently omitted morpheme (goodglass and berko, 1960; de villiers, 1974). these two morphemes were assessed in an attempt to control for the possibility of obtaining too few omissions (exclusive use of plural) or too many omissions (exclusive use of past), for between group com parison. martin et al., (1975) contend that the number of phonemes within a syllable is not significant in aphasic error performance on a given phoneme. johns & darley (1970) suggest that the number of syllables is an important factor in error performance. for these reasons all stem and morpheme combinations were restricted to monosyllabic words of the structure cvc or cvcc. no initial clusters were included an an attempt was made to randomly vary the consonants utilized in initial position. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationship between phonology and infectional morphology in an agrammatic aphasic 5 3. phrase/sentence list of plural, possessive and third person singular in order to compare r.p.'s production of the plural marker, the possessive marker and the third person singular morpheme, a list of 135 sentences/phrases was compiled (appendix iii). the stimuli were divided into nine groups, so that each allophone /s,z,az/ of each morpheme was tested fifteen times. phrases were constructed since the possessive nature of a stimulus cannot be inferred from a single word. for example horse's in a repetition task would be interpreted as a plural. it was felt that a minimum of four syllables was necessary to convey the possessive nature of a stimulus, for example, 'the horse's mouth'. all stimuli therefore comprised four syllables. c. administration of tasks each list was administered using two modes of presentation. 1. an auditory mode — repetition 2. a visual mode reading two modes of presentation were selected because the stringent criteria adopted in test construction limited the number of stimuli available in certain groups. due to the specific nature of the areas being investigated, a spontaneous sample, which may be considered as an ideal medium for linguistic investigation, would not have enabled sufficient sampling of all aspects under study. for repetition tasks, r.p. was instructed to repeat each item after the experimenter. if no response was given the item was repeated. for reading tasks each item was printed clearly and individually in 10mm capital letters. word items were printed on 7cm by 9cm cards and phrase/sentence items on 14cm by 9cm cards. each card was presented singly to r.p. and she was instructed to read it aloud. testing was carried out on two different days for approximately forty-five minute periods in order to control for fatigue. d. analysis procedure and scoring all responses were recorded on a revox tape recorder (model 375 dolby version) and subsequently transcribed in broad phonetic script by three independent transcribers. a two out of three consensus was accepted for each word. analysis procedure specific to particular tasks j i. ccvcc word list j a) a frequency count of correct versus incorrect initial and final clusters in the three categories was carried out. b) phonological errors occurring in final clusters were differentiated according to type, on the basis of two broad categories, namely sequencing and substitution errors. sequencing errors for the purposes of this study included additions, omissions and metatheses. in instances where a number of phonological errors occurred in one cluster, each was tabulated separately. for example, /st/ —» /tz/ was scored as both a sequencing and a substitution error. 2. sonorance-inflection word list a frequency count of morphemes omitted, retained and incorrectly produced was carried out. the incorrect category included instances where r.p. retained a morpheme, but not the particular morpheme under stimulation; for example the allophone /z/ instead of /d/. results were expressed as percentages. 3. phrase/sentence list of plural, possessive and third person singular a frequency count of retained morphemes was carried out. a morpheme was considered as retained even if r.p.'s allophonic realization was not entirely accurate. for example, 'wishes' (third person singular) was realized by r.p. as /wijs/ and this was scored as a retained inflection. results and discussion results of r.p.'s performance on each task will be presented individually and overall trends will be discussed in relation to the stated aims of this study. 1. a comparison of r.p. 's error performance on pc, pcm and mc consonant clusters a) frequency count of correct versus incorrect consonant clusters table 1 frequency count of correct versus incorrect initial and final consonant clusters pc pcm mc initial cluster final cluster initial cluster final cluster initial cluster final cluster no. no. no. no. no. no. correct 39 36 41 22 53 28 incorrect 61 64 59 78 47 72 total (n) 100 100 100 100 100 100 table 2 breakdown of correct initial and final clusters category correct clusters category initial cluster final cluster category no. no. pc 39 36 pcm 41 22 mc 53 28 total 133 86 table 1 illustrates that r.p.'s incorrect final clusters increased in the progression pc —» mc —» pcm. table 2 provides a more specific breakdown of correct clusters. it indicates that within each category, more initial clusters were correctly produced than final clusters. for the purposes of this study, 'difficulty' was conceptualized as the number of incorrect clusters in a category. martin et al., (1975), in a similar study, conceptualized difficulty as the number of phonemic errors in a particular category. for example 'drink' / 'glink' contains two phonemic errors, whereas 'drink' / 'grink' contains one phonemic error. martin et al. contend that the former production reflects greater difficulty than the latter. in this study, any two incorrect clusters were considered as being equally 'difficult', die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 meryl kobrfn and lesley wolk irrespective of the number of phonemic errors occurring in each. results of the present study were not entirely consistent with martin et al.'s, (1975) prediction of increased difficulty in the progression pc pcm — mc. however, the fact that incorrect final clusters increased in the direction pc mc, supports the contention that due to the added cognitive decision component, a ccvcc word with two morphemes (e.g. dressed) would be more difficult for an aphasic to process than a ccvcc word which has one morpheme (e.g. trump) (martin et al., 1975). the feet that the pcm category reflected the highest frequency of incorrect clusters is difficult to explain. it is felt that the pcm category as proposed by martin et al, (1975) needs careful consideration. whether in feet the /st/ cluster in a word such as 'breast', for example, is interpreted as a possible morphological combination by the aphasic, is open to speculation. however, results of the present study, suggest that further research into whether the pcm category is conceptualized as a phonological or morphological construction, and whether such a category is in fact valid, could be of value in providing insight into the interaction between these two linguistic components. the finding that initial clusters are more likely to be correctly produced than final clusters is consistent with that of martin et al., (1975) who contend that the final cluster position may suggest the possibility of a morphological component which would thus pose a more difficult processing task to the aphasic. b) frequency count of sequencing versus substitution error types in final clusters table 3 distribution of phonological errors in final cluster position according to sequencing and substitution error types category of error pc pcm mc category of error no. no. no. sequencing 34 54 54 substitution 58 59 36 total number of errors 92 113 90 table 3 clearly illustrates that the three categories are distinguishable on the basis of error type. the pc category reflects a greater proportion of substitution versus sequencing errors; mc a greater proportion of sequencing versus substitution errors; and pcm an approximately equal distribution of both. the distribution of error types supports the contention that " . . . the substitution error is more indicative of a basic phonological impairment, while sequencing errors are more indicative of interactions between the phonological and morphological components" (martin et al., 1975, p. 446). the approximately equal error distribution in the pcm category, seems to suggest the need for further research into the aphasics' conceptualization of this group as discussed above. 2. frequency count of omitted inflections as a function of the sonorance hierarchy of the preceding segment table 4 represents a summary of morphemes retained, omitted and incorrectly produced, expressed in relation to n. omission of the morpheme increased in the order v (least omitted) s l ν ^ f (most omitted), where (v), (s), (l), (n) and (f), represent the sonorance category of the final segment of the stem! more inflections were retained following vowels than consonants. within the consonantal group, the morpheme was most likely to be omitted when preceded by a fricative and least likely to be omitted when preceded by a stop. kean (1977) hypothesized that omission of the morpheme would increase as the airflow in the articulation of a segment became more impeded, that is in the order v (least omitted) ^ l — » n — » f — » s (most omitted). this contention was not supported by the present results. a possible explanation for the finding that the morpheme is more likely to be retained following a vowel than a consonant may be related to the syllable structure of words included in this task. stem morphemes ending in vowels were of the construction cv (e.g. bee): while those ending in consonants were of the construction cvc (e.g. dog). addition of the morpheme resulted in cvc stimuli for the vowel category (e.g. bees) and cvcc stimuli for the consonant category (e.g. dogs). therefore retention of the inflection when the stem ends in a vowel, and omission when it ends in a consonant, may reflect a strategy to maintain the cvc syllable structure form.' there is thus clear evidence to suggest that this subject has a tendency to employ simplification processes. j i shankweiler and harris (1973) suggest that vowels are easier for aphasics to produce than consonants and that within the consonantable 4 distribution of morphemes retained, omitted or incorrectly produced, expressed as a function of the sonorance hierarchy of the stem final segment sonorance hierarchy of final segment of stem morpheme 1 (v) vowel diphthong ν = 49a (l) liquids ν = 12a (n) nasals ν = 52" (f) fricatives ν = 34a (s) stops ν = 46" retained % 81,63 33,40 36,50 32,30 45,60 omitted % 16,37 41,60 50,00 64,70 32^60 incorrect % 2,00 25,00 13,50 3,00 21,80 total % 100,00 100,00 100,00 100,00 ' 100,00 the number of words in which the final segment produced by r.p. corresponded to the category under investigation. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationship between phonology and infectional morphology in an agrammatic aphasic 7 tal group, fricatives and affricates are more susceptible to error than other phonemes. the present findings suggest that the omission of inflections may be conditioned by the susceptibility to error or 'complexity of articulation' of the preceding segment. it appears that although r.p.'s inflectional omission was not conditioned strictly by the sonorance hierarchy of the preceding segment, omission and retention were influenced by certain phonological characteristics of this segment as well as the overall syllable structure of the word. if her inflectional deletions were solely attributable to a syntactic impairment, an equal percentage of omissions would have been expected across all groups. verification of the present trends on a large group of agrammatic aphasics, assessing a variety of inflectional morphemes, may provide strong evidence for an interaction between phonological and morphological breakdown. 3. frequency count of retained plural, possessive and third person singular morphemes as a function of their stimulus allophonic realization correct. however, the fact that r.p. retained the syllabic allophone /az/ with greater frequency than the non-syllabic form /s,z/ is consistent with the findings of goodglass (1976) and in opposition to those of de villiers (1974). goodglass (1976, p. 250) ascribes the greater retention of the syllabic form /az/ to the added 'saliency' of the extra syllable. he states that "there is no basis at present for anything but a first order intuitive definition of saliency as the resultant of information, load, affective tone, increased amplitude and intonational stress" (goodglass, 1976, p. 253). it is clear that this definition of saliency, includes both receptive and expressive components. therefore, if saliency, as delineated above by goodglass (1976), were the sole explanation for the present findings, greater retention of the voiced /z/ as opposed to the unvoiced /s/ would have been expected, particularly on repetition tasks. however, the fact that r.p. showed greater retention of /s/ as opposed to /z/, suggests that alternative explanations, possibly with phonological implications should be sought. wolk (1978) reported that voiced fricatives may be more susceptible to error in aphasics than their voiceless cognates, which may explain r.p.'s greater retention of the stimulus allophone /s/ as opposed to /z/. liable 5 distribution of retained plural, possessive and third person singular morphemes as a function of their stimulus allophonic realization stimulus allophonic realization plural no. possessive no. third person singular no. total no. /s/ 24 13 14 51 / z / 24 7 8 39 /3z/ 30 17 28 75 total 78 37 50 165 table 5 clearly illustrates that the frequency of morpheme retention increases in the progression: possessive (least retained) —> third person singular —> plural (most j retained). the frequency of allophonic retention increases in the progression /z/ (least retained) —> /s/ —> /az/ (most retained). this pattern is maintained for each individual inflection, with the exception of plurals where /s/ = /z/. an interactional analysis reveals that: — third person singular /az/ is better retained than plurals /s/ and / z / . — possessive /az/ is better retained than third person singular /s/ and /z/. — possessive /s/ is better retained than third person singular /z/. morphological complexity the hierarchy of grammatical difficulty exhibited by r.p. is consistent with reports in the literature (de villiers, 1974;' goodglass, 1976). phonological complexity for the. purposes of the present study, any realization of the allophone was tabulated as a retention of the stimulus allophone. this phonological scoring procedure precluded strict comparison with other writers, who considered the allophone as either correct or inwhilst some explanations have been provided, a more complete account of the above findings would involve detailed consideration of receptive language and perceptual factors, which is felt to go beyond the scope of this study. however, r.p.'s differential retention of the stimulus allophones /s/ and /z/, suggests that further research into receptive language and phonemic perception in agrammatic aphasics, may provide valuable information. interactional analysis the finding that syllabic forms of more complex morphemes are more likely to be retained than non-syllabic forms of less complex morphemes, provides strong evidence for an interaction between apparent phonological and morphological hierarchies of difficulty. major trends overall, the following trends exhibited by r.p. in this study, suggest an interdependence between the phonological and morphological levels of breakdown for this case: la. consonant clusters of purely phonological construction were more likely to be correctly produced than clusters containing a morphological component or suggesting the possibility thereof. b. the cluster categories pc, pcm and mc were clearly differentiated in terms of the proportion of sequencing versus substitution errors. pc reflected a greater proportion of substitution errors, mc a greater proportion of sequencing errors and pcm an approximately equal distribution of both. 2. inflectional deletion appeared to be conditioned by phonological characteristics of the preceding segment as well as the syllabic structure of the word. 3. there was an apparent interaction between the grammatical and phonological hierarchies of difficulty in three morphemes which are homonyms phonologically. conclusions results of this study reflect a mutual interdependence between the phonologically and morphologically impaired systems of this agrammatic aphasic patient. such findings contradict the notions that agrammatism is a uniquely phonological deficit (kean, 1977) or that it is a disruption of the syntactic component of language co-occurring with an independent disorder of articulation (berndt and caramazza, 1981, p. 171). an interactional model between phonology and morphology, suggesting a unitary linguistic representation is strongly die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 meryl kobrin and lesley wolk indicated. verification of the present trends on a large group of agrammatic aphasics may support the contention that there is no single impairment at a specific level in agrammatism. rather, a complex interaction of linguistic processes, all of which are operating at a reduced level of efficiency would be indicated (martin et al., 1975). such a model highlights the inherent limitations of fragmenting the linguistic components in the treatment of agrammatism and suggests a number of clinical implications for the aphasiologist. firstly, diagnostic procedures could possibly include a description of morphological breakdown in the context of phonological breakdown, rather than two detailed but separate analyses. secondly, phonological environments conditioning the omission of inflectional morphemes should be evaluated for each patient and therapy could proceed from phonologically simpler to more complex contexts. further research into the relationship between linguistic components in both aphasia and child language disorders is indicated. this would not only facilitate a more holistic approach to the management of these patients, but would provide greater insight into the organization of language components in a linguistically intact system. references berndt, r.s., & caramazza, a. syntactic aspects of aphasia. in acquired aphasia, m.t. sarno (ed.) new york, academic press, 1981. blumstein, s.e. some phonological implications of aphasic speech. in psycholinguistics and aphasia, h. goodglass & s.e. blumstein (eds.) baltimore, johns hopkins university press, 1973. de villiers, j. quantitative aspects of agrammatism in aphasia. cortex, 10, 36-54, 1974. garman, m. is broca's aphasia a phonological deficit? in linguistic controversies, d. crystal (ed.) london, edward arnold, 1981. goldman, r., & fristoe, m.s. goldman-fristoe test of articulation, minnesota, american guidance service, inc., 1969. goldman, r., fristoe, m.s., & woodcock, r.w. goldman-fristoe test of auditory discrimination. minnesota, american guidance service, inc., 1970. goodglass, h. agrammatism. in studies in neurolinguistics, h. whitaker& h.a. whitaker (eds.) vol. 1, new york: academic press, 1976. goodglass, h., & berko, j. agrammatism and inflectional morphology in english. j. speech hear. res., 3, 257-267, 1960. goodglass, η., & kaplan, e. the assessment of aphasia and related disorders. boston veterans administration hospital and aphasia research centre, department of neurology, boston university, 1972. johns, d.f., & darley, f.l. phonemic variability in apraxia of speech. j. speech hear. res., 13, 556-583, 1970. kean, m.l. the linguistic interpretation of aphasic syndromes: agrammatism in broca's aphasia; an example. cognition, 5 9-46, 1977. kolk, h.h.j. the linguistic interpretation of broca's aphasia. a reply to m.l. kean. cognition, 6, 349-362, 1978. marquardt, t.p., reinhart, j.b., & peterson, h.a. markedness analysis of phonemic substitution errors in apraxia of speech. j. commun. dis., 12, 481-494, 1979. martin, a.d., wasserman, n.h., gilden, l., gerstman, l., & west, j. a. a process model of repetition in aphasia: an investigation of phonological and morphological interactions in aphasic error performance. brain and language, 2 434-450 1975. menyuk, o., looney, p.l. relationships among components of the grammar in language disorders. j. speech hear. res. ,15, 395-406, 1972. paul, r., & shriberg, l. associations between phonology and syntax in speech-delayed children. j. speech hear. res., 25, 536-553, 1982. shankweiler, d., & harris, k.s. an experimental approach to the problem of articulation in aphasia. in psycholinguistics and aphasia, h. goodglass and s.e. blumstein (eds.) baltimore: johns hopkins university press, 1973. wolk, l. a markedness analysis of initial consonant clusters in aphasic phonological impairment: a case study. s.a.j. commun. disord., 25, 81-100, 1978. appendix i ccvcc word list purely phonological construction (pc) blank blanch blink branch breadth frank brink brisk clamp clank clasp clench clinch blimp clump cramp crank crunch brunch crisp phonological construction with morphological possibility (pcm) blind blitz blond blunt brand breast bronze bland brunt cleanse cleft craft crest grant crust crux draft drift flex flux drench drink drunk flank flask shrink french fringe frisk frump grange grasp plank scalp slump stump stink stamp plump prank trump clamp tramp shrimp trunk spunk spank stench trench stomp possibility (pcm) friend front frost grunt gland glance glint craft grand grind ground plant prance prince print scant slant scald spend spoilt stand stance stilt stunt " thrift trance trend trust blast flint phonological construction with morphological combination (mc) frogs tricks spots flossed planes plans slides planned spoons flags sticks groaned steps stoves closed flocks frills spades stocks grabbed stabbed flipped gripped blessed dressed crates braced bricks . smacked crossed brats plaits pricked slapped pressed crabs clocks drapes groped placed cribs clogs ' pricks gripped plates priced clicks grapes clips x the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationship between phonology and infectional morphology in an agrammatic aphasic a p p e n d i x ii sonorance inflection word list vowels and dipthongs liquids nasals izl idl izl idl izl bees lied bills bowled farms fees weighed bulls rolled palms knees cared bells called psalms keys died wills pulled charms peas sighed walls wailed rams bears tied wells sailed lambs pears paid pools piled worms firs toyed pills filed bins ways feared pals ruled buns boys reared shells fooled fins toys wired sales cooled pans goes hired goals peeled sons cows shared girls sealed sins fears dared mills mailed vines shears sheared tills railed signs fricatives stops idl izl idl izl idl bombed caves paved jobs dubbed combed coves saved bibs robbed dimmed hives sieved ribs rubbed roamed wives dived cobs fibbed named dives lived cubs rigged tamed waves raved kegs wagged timed thieves loved pigs sagged shunned revs revved pegs begged pinned fives shaved tags tugged sinned calves shoved figs mugged dined hooves seethed buds bugged moaned leaves heaved dogs jogged fined sieves soothed beds jabbed signed doves . moved rugs pegged shamed lives waved pods jigged a p p e n d i x iii phrase/sentence list of plural, possessive and third person singular plural i si i have big cats give me red mats take the pips out turn the lights off i will buy pots i wear white socks i like my shirts they are white rats i like pet shops he took big sips we both hate bats we have red gates i have two kites i buy eight books give me the sacks possessive isl the cat's big paw pat's little boy kate's big red hat the hat's ribbon the pet shop's door the cup's handle the white rat's tail the pet's delight the cake's icing the lake's water the'rake's handle the book's cover rick's baby girl the pope's red robe the pot's handle third person singular isl the young boy laughs the big dog barks the big boy fights that young man jokes the good boy writes he likes to run he wants to eat she puts it in he takes it out she sips the coke he eats the cake he pats the dog the young man talks the good boy waits the white dog bites plural izl i have big dogs she has two bags i bought the pigs he ate two figs i hurt both legs he made the beds i found the logs i like the jugs i have two jobs i bought two wigs he took two rods pass me the rags here are two rugs • i saw lion cubs i bought two nibs possessive izl the dog's collar the jug's handle bob's little girl dad's new red car the boy's handle the big pig's hoof the bird's one wing the bed's one leg the pub's doorway the lad's new toy the lab's doorway the mob's loud noise rob's new baby the bud's petal the rug's colour third person singular izl he begs for food she rubs her leg he rides the bike he robs the bank pat jogs to work he wags his tail she hugs the boy he lugs the chair she digs a hole she leads the way he reads the book she feeds the dog tim guides the man he bides his time he fibs often plural bz/ two big horses they are nurses two red purses take two buses i like sauces run the races take two paces tie your laces two sad faces two big cases take two wishes two big bushes wash the dishes two long sashes two hard courses possessive hzl the nurse's hat the jones's car the horse's mouth james's big hat liz's red shoe the fish's mouth the witch's nose the boss's car the case's key gus's new car roz's new hat the bus's door dez's new cat bess's red hat madge's new car third person singular hzl he washes it he rushes home he dashes home he wishes once she cashes it he misses her she kisses him it gushes out she bashes him he watches her it mashes food he fishes there she pushes him he lashes out she dishes up die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h i s page is s p o n s o r e d by l i t e r a r y s e r v i c e s ( p t y ) ltd academic & medical booksellers johannesburg: pretoria: durban: cape town: campus bookshop 34 bertha street 2017 braamfontein westdene services 76 king george street 2001 hillbrow westdene medio 36 nedbank plaza 175 beatrix street arcadia pretoria phone: (011) 339-1711 phone: (011) 339-1711 phone: (012) 266366 (012) 3233487 phone: i (031) 253221 ext 22 logans westdene 660 umbilo road 1400 durban westdene rondebosch 18 main road 7700 rondebosch phone: (021) 654830 (021) 692407 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 15 strangers in the house? communication between mothers and their hearing impaired children who sign lavanithum joseph and erna alant centre for augmentative and alternative communication university of pretoria abstract many hearing impaired children rely on signing as a method of communication and are educated through this medium at school. while there is a paucity of information on the use of signing in the home, the impression in the literature is that these children are often unable to communicate through this medium in their homes. this has serious implications for family relationships as well as the personal well being, educational success and social integration of the child. the present study explored the signing experience of 45 mothers of children in the junior primary phase at schools for the deaf in the durban region of kwa-zulu natal as a reflection of the use of signing within the home. a descriptive survey design, using two researcher administered questionnaires, was used to obtain information on the signing practice of mothers, exploring aspects related to the extent to which signing is used, the type of signing used and signing proficiency. the findings revealed mothers' signing skill to be inadequate in terms of their own assessment on rating scales and descriptions of their signing. they signed less frequently than their children did, using speech more often, and with the vast majority using simultaneous communication when they did sign. signing ability was judged to be below that of their children, with sign vocabularies of between 0-50 words for 85% of the participants. it appears that sign learning by hearing parents of deaf children in this region needs to be actively promoted. the implications of these findings are discussed to address the communication needs of signing deaf children and their families within the south african context. key words: sign language, sign systems, signing proficiency, deafness, mothers of deaf children. introduction . ' the professions of 'speech language therapy and / audiology in south africa have concerned themselves with issues of signing for some time. studies have been undertaken to assess the signing abilities of teachers (aron et al., 1986) and resource development for sign learning (cohen, 1996). deaf education and deaf culture in south africa have been raised as pertinent issues (penn and reagan, 1991; penn, 1993). more recently there have been • discussions around the issue of sign bilingualism in deaf education with reference to the profession (joseph, 1998; joseph, 2000; noorbaai, 2000). professional interest in the young deaf child begins during the period of diagnosis and early habilitation and interfaces with deaf education. the greatest task facing both parents and professionals working with deaf children in the early years is that of breaking the communication barrier that exists between a mother and her deaf child (gregory, 1995). parents need to establish communication, as any withdrawal from interaction would later be played out in a complex way by society's withdrawal of communication from the deaf child (hull, 1997). the decisions now facing parents, however, tends to be more complex because of the choice to sign or not to sign, and whether or not to stress signing with speech or sign language (gregory, 1995). parents are given options with regard to communication methods that are available, viz. oralism, total communication or bilingualism (lynas, 1994), yet they often have to enter an education system that focuses on signing. for example, all eight public schools for the deaf in kwa-zulu natal use signing as a medium of instruction. thus parents have to sign out of necessity if they want to communicate with, and be part of their deaf child's world. the complexity· of this situation cannot be underestimated. more than 90% of deaf children are born to hearing parents who do not know how to sign (kyle and woll, 1988). acceptance of signing, learning to sign, and interacting meaningfully and easily with the child through signing is what is required. the signing skills of parents, their attitude towards signing and the opportunities they have to learn to sign are strongly related to each other and have a direct bearing on their use of signing (bornstein, 1990). in this respect there is a paucity of information on the experiences of mothers who sign. there are indications however, that signing is generally not exploited in the home environment. it appears that the communication methods used at school do not commonly match those used in the home, as speech rather than signing seems to dominate interaction among hearing parents and their signing deaf children (bess and humes, 1995). according to bornstein (1990) half the children exposed to signing at school may not see it at home. the majority of deaf children learn to sign without input from their parents and 90% of parents are learning to sign at the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 lavanithum joseph & erna alant same time as their child (gregory and hartley, 1991). luterman (1987) states that most hearing parents do not develop sufficient signing skills to communicate fully with their children. it therefore appears that while there are parents who do learn and use signing to communicate with their deaf signing children, many do not. learning to sign requires an investment in time and effort that many parents are often unwilling and/or unable to make (bornstein, 1990). inadequate signing proficiency of parents of children who are in signing programmes in the education system can be attributed to barriers that parents encounter in their attempts to learn to sign. these can be broadly classified as distance from the learning centre, transport difficulties, time constraints due to employment and family responsibilities, language difficulties with regard to lack of proficiency in the spoken language of the school, lack of immersion in a signing environment, and the lack of resources (swisher and thompson, 1985; cohen, 1996; lynas, 1994). impoverished communication between signing children and their parents is evident in the numerous accounts of the difficulties, isolation and bewilderment that deaf children experience in situations that are considered normal in everyday life. these reports tell of children's inability to access information vital to their understanding of events around them, and of sharing their thoughts, joys and fears with those closest to them (lane et al., 1996; mindel and vernon, 1987). in most instances, these are their parents and siblings. these children and their parents are described as being "strangers" in their own home (mindel and vernon, 1987:xix). the terms being used to describe the failure to address the language needs of deaf children are "oppression" and "abuse" (preston, 1994:127; deafsa, 1997:10). on the other hand, mothers of young deaf children have been reported as feeling "cutoff' from their children, often wondering what their children were thinking or feeling (gregory, 1995:194). mindel and vernon (1987) show concern for the emotional frustrations that deaf children and their parents undergo and call for enhanced sympathy toward these families. information relating to parents' use of signing appears to be particularly relevant at this stage in deaf education. there have been significant changes in the status of sign language all over the world with important implications for deaf education and service provision. sign language has been acknowledged as a legitimate language, equivalent to any spoken language (kyle and woll, 1988). many researchers consider sign language as the first and natural language of deaf people (sacks, 1989; stokoe, 1990). in keeping with these developments, sign language has been accepted by the south african constitution as a language that needs to be promoted, and the new south african schools act recognizes sign language as an official language for educational purposes (deafsa, 1997). bilingualism as an approach in deaf education is being promoted by academics and the world federation for the deaf. this educational philosophy has implications for the professions of speech-language therapy and audiology. these relate particularly to the issues of counselling at diagnosis, prescriptive practice, information dispensing following diagnosis, the role of the audiologist in parentinfant programmes and the role of speech training for the deaf child. it has been stated that parents have not been served well by the professionals involved in the area of deafness (mindel and vernon, 1987) due to a lack of awareness of the communication needs of parents and their deaf children as well as a failure to provide them with realistic solutions. related to this is the critical issue of lack of consensus among professionals in the field with regard to signing being a method of choice with the deaf child. therefore, eliciting information from parents themselves about their signing experience, would appear to contribute to a better understanding of the unique needs of parents who need to sign. the extent to which signing is used within the home context by signing children and their parents would be explored in terms of meaningful communication that is perceived as mainly inadequate by most writers. methodology aim the aim of the study was to describe the signing practice of mothers of junior primary school children in signing programmes at schools for the deaf in the durban metropolitan area. objective: to describe mothers' perceptions of: • their use of signing in comparison to other methods of communication used • the type of signing used • their signing proficiency research design a descriptive survey design was employed with the use of a structured interview format. this method was considered suited to the data which were the perceptions of mothers (leedy, 1993). the design, through the descriptive record allows scrutiny of the data as well as relations within the data, including the use of statistical techniques. subjects subject selection criteria the subjects needed to be mothers of children attending schools for the deaf where signing was used as a medium of instruction. mothers were chosen as they are considered the main language stimulators in the home and the caregivers most likely to be involved in the intervention programme. in addition, mothers were considered to be more easily accessible and, by restricting the sample to mothers alone, the homogeneity of the sample would be increased. legal guardians and primary care-givers such as grandmothers were included in keeping with the definition of parent used in the public discussion document of the national commission on special needs in education and training and the national committee for education support services (1997). only schools in durban registered with the kwa-zulu natal education department were included. children needed to be in the junior primary phase of education as the first level of formal schooling was seen as beneficial in terms of addressing the issue of parent-child communication in the early years of school life. only children in grade one (entry the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) strangers in the house? communication between mothers and their earing impaired children who sign 17 level) and grade three (exit level) were selected as this was considered an adequate range of this phase. thus the sampling method can be described as purposeful sampling. description of schools all four public schools for the deaf in the durban area were included in the study. these schools were based at amanzimtoti, gillits, newlands west and umlazi. two schools were residential schools. all schools were english medium schools. the sign dialect varied amongst the schools, although all four schools embraced the concept of sign bilingualism. description of mothers forty five mothers participated in the study. mothers came from a diversity of backgrounds. age: most mothers (62%) fell in the 30-39 year age group. marital status and employment: a large number of mothers (39%) were single mothers with 52% of mothers working. educational status: most mothers (55%) had not completed high school. language background: more than a third of mothers (36%) spoke zulu in the home, while many of them (28%) were not proficient in the language medium of the school, viz. english. residential background: the majority of mothers (91%) lived in and around durban. home visits revealed that many mothers lived in poor conditions in both informal and formal dwellings, while some came from wealthy suburban homes. description of children there were 42% males and 58% females in the study. age and grade: children were between 5 and 13 years of age, with most of them (33%) in the 8year category. / approximately half the children were in grade one (51%) / and half in grade two (49%). hearing status: according to school records, hearing losses (based on the pure-tone average of the better ear unaided), fell within the range of moderate to profound. most of the children (58%) had profound losses, while 27% ' had severe losses. thus the majority of children (85%) in the study could be classified as having severe and profound hearing impairment,'with onset prelingually (89%). hearing aids were worn consistently (daily at home and at school) by a small percentage of these children (24%), although 91% of them did wear hearing aids. signing background: all children were able to sign. most children (64%) had begun signing between the ages of 3 and 6 years with the majority signing for 3 years or more (78%). the average number of years of signing was 4 years. spoken language: with reference to spoken language ability, 18% were described by their mothers as not using spoken language at all, while a further 58% were only able to communicate using a small vocabulary, speaking on a few topics. thus 76% of the children were considered by their mothers to have inadequate spoken language ability. approximately a third of the children lived in the school residential facility. description of the instrument two questionnaires, questionnaire i-mothers' experience with sign communication: background information, and questionnaire ii-mothers' experience with sign communication: methods of communication were developed based on relevant literature and consultation with experts in the field with regard to the suitability of the questions in terms of structure and content. closedended questions using mainly multiple choice responses were used as it was felt to be an easier format because of the difficulties with terminology and concepts, and their implications for the analysis of responses and reliability of the instrument. care was taken with the wording of questions so as to avoid ambiguity, maintain a logical sequence and provide an exhaustive range of possibilities with the category of "other" available to allow respondents freedom to express themselves. the first questionnaire sought background information of both the mother and the child. the second questionnaire focussed specifically on issues relating to signing practice. questionnaire i consisted of 20 questions that sought demographic information of mothers, mothers' communication background with regard to home language, language proficiency, and hearing status as well as identifying information of the children with regard to gender, age, grade and hearing status. questionnaire ii comprised of 19 questions that pertained to mothers' perceptions of their children's spoken language abilities, methods of communi.cation (speech, natural gestures, signing, writing and 'other') used in parent-child communication in terms of frequency of use and the success of these modes in conveying information, mothers' rating of their own and their children's signing ability, and mothers' description of their signing with regard to range of topics, syntactical complexity and type of signing used. rating scales were used to describe the use of signing in comparison to other methods of communication and to describe signing proficiency. the assessment of signing proficiency was based on the 5point scale used by metz et al. (1997) in the language background questionnaire. according to the authors, the rating of signing skills on a scale of one to five, with one being equivalent to no skills and five being equivalent to excellent skills, was found to have reasonable criterion validity for self-assessment of sign language skills of adults as there was a high degree of congruence between self-assessed communication skills and formal measures of communication skills. the authors further cite the advantages of self -assessment instruments as including ease of administration, low expense, non-threatening and non-invasive nature, as well as validity of the tool in terms of face validity as self reports reflect personal experience. mothers rated their signing proficiency both globally and specifically on a receptive level. their global skill was taken as the main indicator of their signing skill. information was also sought on the communication modes and signing proficiency of the children for purposes of comparison as parents have been observed to adjust their level of communication to suit that of their children (gallaway and richards, 1994). multiplechoice and multiple response questions were used to allow parents to describe the type of signing they used. a cross check was used to assess the validity of the response by probing with the use of the centre for assessment and demographic studies (cads) measure die suid-afrikaanse tdskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 to assess the type of signing used by teachers, based on the way teachers described how they would sign two english sentences (woodward, 1990). both a pre-pilot and a pilot study were undertaken to test the instrument and assess the conditions under which the study would be carried out. following these procedures adjustments were made to the wording of certain questions to improve clarity. the questionnaires were translated into zulu as many children at the schools came from a zuluspeaking background. the back-translation method was used. note: this article is based on an aspect of a study (joseph, 1998) which may be consulted for a sample of the questions described above. procedure mothers were accessed via the school. school principals granted permission for letters requesting parent participation to be sent home via the children selected for the study, viz. all children in grades one and grades three at the selected schools. interviews were set up with all parents who indicated their willingness to participate in the study via return slips. interviews were confirmed telephonically. the site of the interview was determined by parent preference such as the home, place of employment or school to encourage parent participation. interviews were scheduled over a twoweek period and were of approximately 20 minutes duration. questionnaires were administered via a structured interview by the researcher or a fieldworker who was fluent in zulu and english. fieldworkers were chosen for their familiarity with the interview process. two fieldworkers were used. one fieldworker was a nurse at one of the schools and who had frequent contact with parents and often acted as an interpreter at the school. the other fieldworker was a student in her third year of study in speech and hearing therapy and who had experience in interviewing parents and issues of communication. both field workers received training in terms of administering the particular questionnaires via role-play and during the pilot study so as to ensure reliability and consistency across interviews. the interview was particularly important in obtaining data from those who were illiterate or who had low literacy levels and whose participation would be restricted by the use of a selfadministered questionnaire. data analysis lavanithum joseph & erna alant results and discussion the signing practice of mothers is described in terms of three aspects, viz. their use of signing in comparison to other modes of communication used, the type of signing used, and their signing proficiency. use of signing in comparison to other modes of communication the frequency of signing is presented both in terms of a comparison between its use with other modes of communication, and a comparison between its use by mothers and their children. an overview of communication modes used is presented in table 1. figure 1-3 describes the findings for specific modes. use of signing children tended to sign more frequently than their mothers did as more than half the sample (53%) always used signing as a method of communication, compared to 40% of mothers who always used signing as a method, as is reflected in figure 1. this is further exemplified by the mean responses on table 1 which shows children as bordering on the category of "usually" using signing table 1: frequency of use of the various modes of communication used by mothers and their children (note: the 5 point rating scale used translated as 1 = never, 2 = rarely, 3 = sometimes, 4 = usually and 5 = always). communication mode mother child communication mode x sd x sd signing 3.60 1.44 3.89 1.35 speech 3.69 1.51 3.42 1.57 gestures 3.49 1.38 3.51 1.29 ; writing 1.40 0.72 1.40 0.78 . other 1.46 0.87 1.36 0.81 , the data was analysed using both descriptive and inferential statistics. measures of central tendency (mean, median and mode) and measures of variability (standard deviation and range) were calculated for all variables in questionnaire i and questionnaire ii. correlation coefficients were calculated to determine the strength of the relationship between variables. a series of non-parametric tests including chisquare, spearman's correlation coefficient and mann-whitney tests were used in accordance with the nature of the data for which relationships were sought (leedy, 1993). 60 50 40 30 20 10 0 53 13 22 18 _ 1 7 i n 16 n e v e r r a r e l y s o m e t i m e s u s u a l l y a l w a y s i m o t h e r s ® c h i l d r e n figure 1: comparison of mothers' and children's use of signing the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) strangers in the house? communication between mothers and their earing impaired children who sign 19 compared to mothers who were closer to the description of "sometimes" using signing. there was, however, a positive correlation (r = .7020, ρ < .01) between mothers' and children's use of signing. thus, as the child's tendency to sign increased so did their mother's signing. this tendency to match the child's mode of communication is reported by gregory et al. (1995) in their study of deaf young people and their families. parents whose children had been educated in the oral tradition had subsequently learned to sign or accompanied gesture with their speech if their children were using sign language in their later years. use of speech there was a positive and significant relationship between mothers' use of speech and the child's use of speech (r = .6613, p< .01). once again parents and children tended to match the communication mode used. however, a comparison between mothers' use of signing and speech indicated a negative correlation (r = -.4584, ρ < .01). it therefore appears that parents who were speaking tended not to sign or vice versa. in addition, for those mothers who used speech to communicate, only about a third (31%) found it always successful in conveying information to their child (results not presented). on the other hand, 22% of children who used speech were considered to be always successful in conveying information to their mothers. it therefore appears that speech was used by the majority of mothers despite it not always being successful. the use of speech by mothers of deaf children has been encouraged in both the oral and total communication approaches. gregory (1995) reports that while many mothers in the oral tradition (33% in her study) spoke constantly to their child whether they were listening or not, none reported not speaking at all. preston (1994) confirms this by stating that even those deaf persons who were oral were no more likely to have good communication with hearing parents. only 13% (37 of 288) of his sample of deaf adults could communicate well with at least one parent. use of writing and other methods of communication writing as a method of communication, was generally not exploited by both parents and children. the majority of parents and children (71%) did not use writing at all as a method of communication. those who did use it as a method tended to do so rarely. this could be considered a low result given that children were in the formal phase of schooling, with approximately half the children (49%) in grade three where writing was being used in the educational programme. this is especially so given the difficulties that children, who used speech, were generally having in conveying and receiving information. studies have shown that reading as a receptive mode is highly successful in communicating with deaf children (grove, et al., 1984). the failure to explore writing has implications for the bilingual approach which promotes the use of writing as a primary method of communication to be used in interaction. gregory et al. (1995) also report parents' failure to use writing to communicate with young deaf people. only 1% (1 in 82) of parents reported using writing to communicate. similarly a large number of parents (76%) did not use any other method to communicate. those mothers who did use other methods, did so infrequently and used drawings (11%), pictures (7%), objects (4%) and the "sign language" dictionary (2%) to communicate. over one third of the children (38%) used other methods to communicate and these included the use of drawings (40%), pictures (4%) and the "sign language" dictionary (2%). it appears therefore that there was a group of parents and children who felt the need to explore alternate means of communication as they could not communicate effectively with signing, gestures or speech. the fact that 40% of children 60 « 5 0 1 j? 40 | 30£ 20 ί ο η 0 13 18 11 13 m l 18 20 9 7 never rarely sometimes usually always i mothers id children use of gestures figure 2: comparison of mothers' and children's use of speech in terms of the relationship between mothers' use of gestures and signing, there was a positive correlation (r = .5728, ρ < .01). this is unlike the relationship with mothers' use of speech and signing. it therefore appears that mothers who used formal signing more also tended to use gestures more. however, only 39% of mothers who used gestures found it always successful in conveying information. the need to use gestures seems to indicate a possible inadequacy of their formal signing skill as well as an inability to communicate through speech. , ν cn η c a υ l. a q. 40 35 30 25 2 0 15 1 0 5 0 31 31 11 11 11 ε 18 u l n e v e r rarely s o m e t i m e s u s u a l l y a l w a y s i m o t h e r s h c h i l d r e n figure 3: comparison of mothers' and children's use of gestures die suid-afrikaanse tdskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 lavanithum joseph & erna alant were using drawing to communicate indicates their need to establish effective communication with their mothers. this finding seems to support the reports of many deaf children of the poor communication between themselves and family members. lane et al. (1996) cites a survey at a school for the deaf which found that only one in ten parents could communicate with his/her deaf child. in summary it is clear that the three most common methods of communication used in parent-child interaction were speech, signing and gestures. however, the extent to which each was used varied much between both mothers and children as is evident in the means recorded in table 1. however, children tended to sign more than their mothers. on the other hand, mothers tended to speak more than their children did. this finding has been reported by bess and humes (1995) as a general trend with children in signing programmes. the reliance on spoken language as the sole means of communication with a deaf child is said to severely restrict parent-child interaction and interfere with the bonding process and communication with far reaching implications for the child's cognitive, linguistic, emotional and educational progress (lane et al., 1996). mothers'description of their signing mothers' signing is described in terms of their stated vocabulary, their communication at discourse level, use of sentences and the type of signing used. sign vocabulary if signing vocabulary by itself was to be used as a yardstick for measuring parents signing skill, the overall results are very low. the majority of mothers (65 %) had a vocabulary ranging from 0 to 20 words. a further 20% had a vocabulary of between 21 to 50 words. in effect, 85% of the sample had a vocabulary size below 51 words. it appears that the majority of mothers had a signing vocabulary that was inadequate for fluent communication table 2: mothers' estimation of their signing vocabulary through the medium of signing. the 50-word vocabulary is regarded as an indication of normal developmental level for a speaking child of 18 months, while the average preschool child, according to yoshinaga-itano as cited in jones (1995), is said to have a vocabulary of between 3000 and 6000 words. in addition sign language lexicons have been described as ranging from 1000 to 4000 words, which, even so, is considered a gross underestimation of american sign language which is considered to be a fully expressive language (bellugi, 1980). the signed english sign system has a lexicon of over 3100 words. the inadequacy of the stated vocabulary of most parents is indicated by the fact that the majority (74%) of those parents who did sign described their signing vocabulary as allowing them to communicate on only a few topics, while none were able to sign on any topic as reflected in figure 4. communication at discourse level the ability to construct sentences is required to communicate efficiently at discourse level. however, only 42% (16) of the 38 mothers who signed could sign sentences. results are similar to those obtained by bornstein (1990) who reported low signing skill at sentence level in a study conducted at maryland school for the deaf in the usa. approximately half the parents could sign sentences after six months of lessons. however, if the entire sample of this study were considered it can be said that only 29%, less than a third, of the sample could sign sentences. of those who could sign sentences, the majority (81%) only felt competent to interpret for their own child. none felt competent to communicate with deaf adults. figure 5 reflects parents' rating of their communication at discourse level. the ability to communicate with deaf adults is considered to be an indication of a high level of skill and is used as a measure of proficiency in sign learning programmes. as none of the parents could communicate freely with a deaf adult, it can be assumed that mastery of signing as a mode of communication had not occurred for any of the parents in the sample. it is acknowledged though, that the level of skill is not the only factor influencing proficiency. sign dialect could also influence whether or not parents feel competent in communicating with deaf adults. however, this factor was not considered significant in the study as parents were expected to be at least in contact with those who shared a similar dialect because of association with the particular school. j size of vocabulary number (n=45) percentage 0 7 16% 1-10 13 29% 11-20 9 20% 21-50 9 20% 51-100 1 2% 101-200 4 9% 201-300 0 0% 301-500 1 2% 501-999 0 0% 1000+ 1 2% total 45 100% most topics 1 0 % \ any topic y g 0 % m a n y topics__ / λ 1 1 6 % ^ ^ • , f e w topics 7 4 % figure 4: mothers' ability to sign on a range of topics the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 47 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) strangers in the house? communication between mothers and their searing impaired children who sign 21 type of signing used home signs constituted the most common type of signing used, with 58% of signing mothers reporting its use. two zuluspeaking mothers who used home signs exclusively were included in this category. one of the mothers reported an elaborate signing system that included a vocabulary of more than 100 signs that allowed her to converse with and interpret for her child. lane et al. (1996) make reference to this type of communication which develops when a child is unable to communicate with family members through an oral medium and does not have access to a formal signing system. home signs are said to quickly disappear when children are exposed to formal signing (stokoe, 1990). it therefore appears that this mother did not have access to the system her child was using at school. the fact that many mothers were using home signs together with formal systems seems to indicate inadequate proficiency in formal signing. fingerspelling was the second most common type of signing used, even though it was used by a relatively small percentage of parents, i.e. 38 %, who were able to sign the manual alphabet. as fingerspelling supplements signing, it appears that the majority of parents would have difficulty in providing novel information to their children as is accommodated with fingerspelling for those who sign. as fingerspelling is a component of both sign languages and sign systems, parents' lack of this skill could be used to estimate their general signing ability. on the probe of the description of how they would sign two english sentences which could be taken as a more accurate representation of the type of signing that mothers were using, 44% (7) of those mothers who could sign at sentence level were using manually coded english (mce). this closely matched the number of mothers who actually stated using this method as reflected in table 3. however, only 19% (3) used key word signing. thus the other 9 mothers who described themselves as using key word signing (table 3) could not be classified in this category, confirming their lack of knowledge in describing their signing or possibly their poor signing skill. gregory et al. (1995) also record the tendency of parents to describe themselves as signing despite poor skills, which their rating 4 rating 3 0 % rating 1 19% v rating 2 81% rating 1 can hold a simple conversation rating 2 can interpret a simple conversation for their deaf child rating 3 can hold a good conversation with a deaf adult rating 4 can interpret for deaf adults without difficulties young deaf children chose to rather not consider as signing at all when describing their parents' ability to sign. it appears that deaf children used stricter criteria for what constituted signing ability. only 1 mother (6%) appeared „ to be using sign language. there were 5 mothers (31%) who could not be classified into a particular system as a result of inconsistencies in their signing. it therefore appears that only 8 of the mothers signed fluently within a prescribed system, that is, 18% of the sample and 50% of those mothers who signed at sentence level. whatever the type of signing used though, it became evident that signing was largely based on english as the vast majority of the 40 mothers who did sign, i.e. 36 (90%), reported speaking and signing at the same time. according to lane et al. (1996:269), 90% of teachers surveyed in a 1990 study in the usa reported using simultaneous speech and signing, with over 85% of teachers using or trying to use mce. it would therefore appear that parents' responses in this study are similar to those of teachers of the deaf. it must be noted though, that the use of simultaneous communication has been associated with poor signing skill (erting, 1980). furthermore, it has been a contention that simultaneous communication does not serve the purpose of communicative interaction (sacks, 1989; lane et al., 1996). mothers'perception of their signing proficiency a comparison was made between mothers' and children's signing skill to ascertain if mothers' level of signing was in any way influenced by their children's level of skill. it is clear from their description of their signing proficiency that mothers' signing skill is inadequate in the main. a large percentage of mothers (29%) stated that they had no signing skill as such while a further third (33%) described themselves as having poor skills. only 5 mothers (11%) considered themselves better than average. these results are in keeping with the previous description of generally low vocabulary levels and inability to converse at discourse level. on the probe of their receptive signing ability, mothers rated their receptive signing higher than expressive signing (as depicted in table 4). this therefore indicates that although mothers described low levels of signing competence, they were able to understand their child's table 3. mothers' report of the type of signing used figure 5: mothers' rating of their communication at discourse level (n=16) type of number percentage signing used (n-40) not sure/unknown 19 48% home-signs 23 58% fingerspelling 15 38% key word signing 14 35% . manual code of english 6 15% south african sign language 3 8% other 14 35% die suid-afrikaanse tvdskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 lavanithum joseph & erna alant signing better than they were able to sign themselves. this seems to indicate that they were realistic in their assessment of their own skills as receptive language is generally judged to be better than expressive language for competent speakers of a language (bornstein, 1990). their responses do differ, however, from findings for hearing teachers of the deaf in non-residential, nonintegrated school settings who in the 1985 cads study (bornstein, 1990) rated their receptive signing poorer than their expressive signing. it therefore appears that these hearing parents were closer in their responses to teachers of the deaf in residential schools who tended to rate their receptive signing higher, perhaps reflecting a better proficiency and maybe perception of their abilities due to increased contact with signers and exposure to signing. it would appear also that children were signing despite their parents' poor proficiency. when a comparison of mothers' and children's signing is made, it is evident that mothers considered their children's signing to be superior to their own, as evidenced by the mean score of 3.7 for children, a rating of almost good and 2.2 for mothers, a rating close to poor. it therefore appears that these mothers are generally unable to communicate on an equal level with their children through signing, which is of serious consequence, if signing is the main medium of communication for the child. this could very well be the case as the majority of children in the study (76%) were described as either not using spoken language or able to speak on only a few topics. this seems to confirm the impression in the literature that signing children are generally unable to communicate within the family. the study by gregory et al. (1995) of young deaf people and their families records only half the number of young people whose preferred language was british sign language as having parents who used any type of signing. to explore possible factors influencing mothers'signing proficiency, relationships with certain demographic details was sought. results are reflected in table 5. it appears that home language was a significant factor. given the fact that the majority of zulu-speakers, 27% of the entire sample, described themselves as not being proficient in english which was the medium of instruction at these schools, it appears that this is impacting on their signing skill. christensen (1986) highlights the difficulty that monolingual spanish speaking parents of children attending english medium schools in the usa have in learning to sign. christensen therefore suggested a trilingual approach as an alternative to bridging the communication gap between english speaking teachers, spanish speaking parents and signing children. the trilingual approach which consisted of using both the oral languages and conceptual signing was subsequently tried in a two year programme using television as a medium. results mdicated that those parents who viewed the programme regularly improved their signing competence (christensen, 1986). in addition, positive attitudinal change was noted both towards hearing loss generally as well as toward com60 -, 50 40 i 30 η 1) m> « 1) ph 2 0 10 0 munication with the child. thus mothers' signing skill was judged to be inadequate in terms of their own assessment on the rating scale and descriptions of their signing. only 11% (5) considered their signing to be good or excellent. the majority of parents had a vocabulary of 50 words or less. as these children are in the first phase of formal schooling and are very much dependent on parent input, the role of parents as the primary educators of their children comes into question. it is evident that the majority of parents in the study are not in a position to support the academic programme as their children are ahead of them in relation to signing skill. as the majority of children were described as having low spoken language ability, it appears that communicative interaction for the majority of mothers and children is inadequate. there is reference in the literature to the signing child growing apart from his family. gregory et al. (1995) describe mothers who began learning to sign in order to be part of their growing child's social world. when language barriers between parents and their young child exist, communication is said to become frustrating and stressful as well as superficial (schoenwald-oberbeck, 1984). it appears therefore that the subjects in this study are at risk for such a situation. there have been other indications that the signing skills of parents of deaf children in other regions in south africa is low, e.g. lenasia (cohen, 1996). it therefore appears that sign learning by parents of deaf children is a serious issue especially within this region if not nationally. while schools for the deaf have traditionally carried the responsibility of teaching parents to sign, it appears that a broader response to the issue is needed, involving close co-operation table 4: mother's rating of their own and their children's signing skill (scores on the rating scale were interpreted as: 1 = no skills, 2 = poor skills, 3 = fair, 4 = good, 5 = excellent) variable x sd mothers' global singing ability 2.22 1.04 mother's receptive singing ability 2.60 1.01 children's signing ability 3.73 0.92 53 33 29 i i no skills poor 16 fangood excellent mothers' global signing ability • children's global signing ability figure 6: comparison of mothers' and children's signing skill the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) strangers in the house? communication between mothers and their hearing impaired children who sign between the health and education sectors as well as welfare and non-governmental organisations. sign learning by hearing parents of deaf children has to be actively promoted at all levels by all professionals working with children who are dependent on a visual mode of communication. policies and strategies need to be put in place to ensure that opportunities are available, together with the necessary support for families to learn to communicate through sign language with young deaf members of the family. conclusion the low levels of signing proficiency among hearing mothers of deaf children in signing programmes has implications for both service provision and future research. fundamental to this would be the stance that parents need to sign and should receive the necessary support when their child is committed to a signing educational programme. as the children in this study were already committed to a methodology, viz. signing, the debates around methodology become less significant in comparison to the issue of parents' communicating meaningfully with their children and supporting the educational programme by becoming competent signers. luterman (1987) stresses that professionals recognize that they are working with functional families that could be made more optimal by their intervention. according to mcconkey (1985), the implication for all professionals working with helping children is that parental help is necessary if they are to do their jobs efficiently and effectively. he does feel however, that this is "more often preached than practiced" (mcconkey, 1985:13). according to jones (1995) family-professional relationships need to be based on fundamental beliefs that make them workable, such as the belief that families have the capacity to be competent and do have resources. the professional role becomes one of mobilizing and assisting families to draw on their resources. outcomes are then based on the professional's capacity and desire to promote the family and utilize their resources. the role of the professional 7 therefore is expanded beyond the technical skills of their discipline, in that professionals drive policies and procedures that result in the empowerment of the family. these principles appear applicable to the audiologist and speech -language therapist who especially in the initial stages, work closely with deaf children and their parents in order to break the communication barrier and prevent isolation of these children within the family. references aron, m. l., louis, r. e. & willemse, j. l. (1986). the issue of signs and the coding of prefix markers by teachers at a school for the deaf. south african journal of communication disorders, 33, 64-72. bellugi, u. (1980). how signs express complex meanings. in c. baker & r. batterson. sign language and the deaf community. usa: national association for the deaf. bess, f. h. & humes, l. e. (1995). audiology: the fundamentals. (2nd ed.) baltimore: williams & wilkins. bornstein, h. (ed.). (1990). manual communication: implications for education. washington: gallaudet university press. christensen, κ. m. (1986). conceptual sign language acquisition by spanish-speaking parents of hearing impaired children. american annals of the deaf, 131, 4, 285-287. cohen, b. (1996). the development of a sign language resource for parents of deaf children in lenasia. unpublished undergraduate research report. johannesburg: university of the witwatersrand. deafsa. (1997). higher education and the needs of the deaf community: discussions with the university of south africa (unisa). unpublished paper, johannesburg. erting, c. (1980). sign language and communication between adults and children. in baker, c. and batterson, r. sign language and the deaf community: essays in honour of william stokoe. silverspring: national association for the deaf gallaway, c. & richards, b. j. (1994). input and interaction in language acquisition. new york: cambridge university press. gregory, s. (1995). deaf children and their families (2nd ed). cambridge: cambridge university press. gregory, s., bishop, j. & sheldon, l. (1995). deaf young people and their families: developing understanding. cambridge: university press. gregory. s. & hartley, g.m. (eds.). (1991). constructing deafness. london: pinter publishers limited. grove, c. & rodda, m. (1984). receptive communication skills of hearing impaired students: a comparison of four methods of communication. american annals of the deaf, 129,4, 378-385. hull, r. h. (1997). aural rehabilitation: serving children and adults (3rd ed). san diego: singular publishing group. jones, a. (1995). joining the family. seminars in hearing, 16, 2, 196-205. joseph, l. (1998). perceptions of mothers of children in schools for the deaf in durban with reference to the use of sign language. unpublished master's dissertation. pretoria: variable mean rank ρ value significance grade grade 1 -21.20 grade 3 -24.89 .3258 not significant day versus residential scholar day scholar -23.17 residential -21.07 .5996 not significant home language english -26.57 zulu -16.03 .0057 significant at <.05 level gender male -20.95 female -24.64 .3288 not significant table 5: exploration of selected variables associated with signing proficiency die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 lavanithum joseph & erna alant university of pretoria. joseph, l. (2000). bilingual education for deaf children: the challenge to the professions of speech-language pathology and audiology. paper delivered at the 2nd international symposium on communication disorders in multilingual populations. 1821 july. pilanesberg national park. south africa. kyle, j. g. & woll, b. (1988). sign languagethe study of deaf people and their language. new york: cambridge university press. lane, h., hoffmeister, r. & bahan, b. (1996). a journey into the deaf-world. california: dawnsignpress. leedy, p. d. (1993). practical research: planning and design. (5th ed.). new york: mcmillan publishing company. luterman, d. (1987). deafness in the family. boston: college-hill press. lynas, w. (1994). communication options in the education of deaf children. london: whurr publishers ltd. mcconkey, r. (1985). working with parents: a practical guide for teachers and therapists. london: croomhelm. metz, d. e, caccamise, f. & gustafson, m. s. (1997). criterion validity of the language background questionnaire : a selfassessment instrument. journal of communication disorders, 30, 1, 23 -32. mindel, e. d & vernon, c. (eds.). (1987). they grow in silence: understanding deaf children and adults. boston: collegehill press national commission on special needs in education and training (ncsnet) &'national committee for education support services (ncess). (1997). education for all. from 'special needs and support' to developing quality education for all learners. summary of publication document. noorbaai, k. (2000). the role of the speech therapist in a school for the deaf. paper delivered at the 2nd international symposium on communication disorders in multilingual populations. 18-21 july. pilanesberg national park. south africa. penn, c. (1993). signs of the times: deaf language and culture in south africa. south african journal of communication disorders, 40, 11-23. penn, c. & reagan, t. g. (1991). towards a national policy for deaf education in the 'new' south africa. south african journal of communication disorders, 38, 19-25. preston, p. (1994). mother father deaf: living between sound and silence. cambridge: harvard university press. sacks, o. w. (1989). seeing voices. a journey into the world of the deaf. texas: pro-ed. inc. schoenwald-oberbeck, b. (1984). a communication programme , for enhancing interaction in families with a hearing impaired child. american annals of the deaf, 129, 4, 362-369. stokoe, w. c (1990). language, prelanguage and sign language. seminars in speech and language, 11, 2, 92-99 swisher, μ. v. & thompson, m. (1985). mothers learning simultaneous communication: the dimensions of the task. american annals of the deaf, 130, 3, 212-217. woodward, j. (1990). sign english in education of deaf students. in bornstein, h. (ed.). manual communication: implications for education. washington: gallaudet university press. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 71 burnout: a smouldering problem amongst south african speechlanguage pathologists and audiologists? marilyn swidler & eleanor ross department of speech pathology & audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract the present study aimed to investigate the frequency of burnout occurring within the profession of speech-language pathology and audiology and to examine possible work situation factors unique to the profession, that may be correlated with burnout. a cross-sectional postal survey research design was used, including a standardised measure of burnout as well as a questionnaire inquiring about demographic and work situation variables. subjects were randomly drawn from names obtained from the south african medical and dental council. results were analysed statistically using pearsons correlations, multivariate analysis as well as bonferroni t-tests. the main finding that emerged was that, as a group speech-language pathologists (splp's) and audiologists in south africa are experiencing moderate levels of emotional exhaustion, low levels of depersonalisation as well as high levels of personal accomplishment. therapists working in hospitals, mainly treating peripheral hearing disorders, working either as audiologists only or speechlanguage therapists and audiologists, with heavy caseloads and extensive paperwork, and perceiving themselves to be under large amounts of work pressure, appear to be the most susceptible to burnout. opsomming die huidige studie het ten doel om die frekwensie van uitbrand ("burnout") binne die beroep van spraak-taalpatologie en oudiologie te ondersoek en om moontlike werksituasiefaktore wat uniek is aan die beroep en met uitbrand gekorreleer kan word, uit te lig. 'n veldopname navorsingstegniek is gebruik tesame met 'n gestandaardiseerde uitbrandmeting asook 'n vraelys aangaande demografiese en werksituasie veranderlikes. proefpersone is toevallig geselekteer uit name van beroepslede wat van die suid-afrikaanse mediese en tandheelkundige raad verkry is. resultate is statistics geanaliseer. die belangrikste bevinding was dat spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe in suid-afrika as 'n groep redelike vlakke van emosionele uitputting, lae vlakke van persoonlikheidsaftakeling, asook hoe vlakke van persoonlike bekwaamheid ervaar. terapeute wat in hospitale werksaam is en veral die wat perifere gehoorafwykings behandel, wat as oudioloe of as spraak-taalterapeute en oudioloe werksaam is, wat swaar pasient-ladings en uitgebreide administratiewe skryfwerk hanteer, en wat hulself ag as om onder groot werksdruk te wees, blyk die mees vatbaar vir uitbrand te wees. j the term " b u r n o u t " was originally coined by freudenberger in 1974 and was subsequently defined by maslach (1982) as a syndrome which results in a state of emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation, and reduced personal accomplishment among individuals who work with people. although burnout has seldom, if ever, been documented in the research literature on south african speech-language pathologists (splp's) and audiologists, it seemed unlikely that these professionals would be immune from this phenomenon— particularly given the stressful nature of their work with communicatively-impaired persons, and the unique socio-economic and politico-cultural context in which they are expected to practise their profession. cherniss (1980), summarises burnout as a process beginning with excessive and lengthy levels of job stress, leading to strain in the worker, i.e. feelings of tension, irritability, fatigue and emotional exhaustion. the second stage of the process occurs when workers defensively cope with the job stress by detaching themselves from the job psychologically, developing depersonalised attitudes and becoming apathetic, cynical and rigid towards their clients. the final stage occurs when caregivers develop a sense of failure and inadequacy regarding their inability to relate to clients and experience a sense of reduced personal accomplishment. the symptoms or effects of burnout can be classified into three dimensions: physical effects (e.g. headaches, insomnia, gastro-intestinal disturbances and susceptibility to illnesses); psychological effects (e.g. emotional exhaustion, negative attitude changes and apathy); and behavioural effects(e.g. lower work productivity, absenteeism, drug and alcohol abuse and an assumed deterioration of patient care (maslach & jackson, 1981; van der ploeg, van leeuwen & kwee, 1990). in addition to the effects on the individual person, the burnout sufferer's family may be affected in terms of increased friction and tension in the home (barling, 1986). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 marilyn swidler & eleanor ross finally, burnout may affect employing organisations which receive poor work performance and low productivity from their employees and have to deal with high rates of absenteeism and staff turnover. it can thus be seen that burnout has individual, institutional and familial repercussions thereby affecting the whole of society. furthermore, from an ecological perspective, burnout is viewed as an imbalance between environmental demands and people's stress-coping resources, and causes or correlates of burnout may occur anywhere within the ecological system including the individual person, the organisation or work environment, and the broader socio-cultural milieu. the person individual factors and personality characteristics play a role in the burnout process. for example, age, gender, marital status and the number of years working experience have been correlated with burnout (bernstein, 1992). burnout among human service workers has been found to be significantly correlated with neurotic anxiety, unrealistic goals and low self-esteem (ratcliff, 1988). furthermore research has correlated burnout with personality factors such as locus of control, hardiness, and type a personality. for example, cherniss (1980) suggested that persons with an external locus of control, i.e. people who believe that their lives are influenced by external forces, were more likely to experience burnout; while kobasa (1979) maintained that persons with high levels of hardiness tended to be resistant to stress because their personalities were characterised by a commitment to personal values, a sense of control over their lives, and a view of change as a challenge. type a behaviour is the term used to describe people who are intensely ambitious, hard-driving, impatient and aggressive with a keen sense of time urgency. although people of this type tend to be highachievers, they tend to experience more heart attacks and be more susceptible to stress than the placid, relaxed type β person (cooper and davidson, 1991). in addition to personal attributes, factors within the work environment can also potentially contribute to burnout. the work environment cherniss (1980), believes that differences in jobs and organisations are stronger etiological factors of burnout than are differences in individual characteristics or personality. he is supported by maslach (1982), who believes that although personality plays a part in burnout, the phenomenon is best understood and modified in terms of situational sources of job related, inter-personal stress. maslach (1982) believes that burnout is a unique form of stress, in that it is a response to the chronic emotional strain of working extensively with other human beings, particularly when they are experiencing problems. splp 's and audiologists do not only deal with communication and hearing problems, they deal with people who also exhibit emotional and physical problems which need to be dealt with in therapy and which can be accompanied by possible negative attitudes on the part of patient and therapist. moreover therapeutic work is usually demanding and emotionally draining. it is difficult to assess the degree of therapeutic success in the helping professions and how much time is required to see progress in therapy. most human service work, compared with other types of work, offers very little feedback, thus the therapist must work in the dark, not knowing the success of his or her efforts. a lack of therapeutic success has been cited as one of the most stressful aspects of therapeutic work (maslach, 1982). miller and potter (1982), investigated stress among a sample of 123 members of the american speech-language-hearing association. they found that 43% of their respondents reported themselves to be moderately burned out. a survey conducted by van der gaag (1988), as cited by kersner and stone (1991), found that 99% of district speech therapy managers in the united kingdom felt that stress levels in the profession had increased in the last few years. moreover, this problem was cited as a major factor affecting the performance of the speech therapists. according to fimian, lieberman and fastenau (1991), who formulated a burnout scale specifically for speech-language pathologists working in schools found that increasing demands for accountability, together with large caseloads, excessive paperwork, and feelings of isolation, would appear to make splp 's and audiologists especially prone to stress and burnout. these researchers point out that splp's cannot be expected to function effectively if suffering from excessive fatigue, headaches and other reported symptoms of stress. it is of interest that fimian et al., (1991, p.444), found stress scores in their sample, to be highly correlated with "bureaucratic restrictions, time and workload management and lack of professional supports". kersner and stone (1991), found that speech therapists as compared to occupational and physical therapists in the united kingdom experienced the size of their caseload and feelings of isolation as stressful. in fact a very high proportion of their respondents, namely 82%, reported being stressed at work. the findings from these three overseas studies of stress and burnout within the profession of speech-language pathology highlight the need for splp's and audiologists' stress symptoms to be taken seriously and for the underlying etiologies to be addressed. these findings also lend support to cherniss's contention that factors within the work environment play a crucial role in the development of burnout. j among the work setting variables that have been implicated in previous research as possible correlates or moderators of the burnout syndrome are social support, workload and clientele. 1 cobb (1976) defines social support as information leading the individual to believe that: 1) he or she is cared for and loved (emotional support), 2) he or she is esteemed or valued (esteem support), and 3) he or she belongs to a network of communication and mutual obligation (network support). according to the buffering hypothesis, as cited by ross, altmaier and russell (1989) , persons involved in supportive relationships are able to rely on others to help them when dealing with stress, a resource likely to lessen the adverse effect of stress on physical and mental health. several researchers cited by macfarlane (1990)^ emphasised that social support from co-workers and supervisors is one of the most effective ways to alleviate negative effects of organisational stress.-the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) burnout: a smouldering problem 73 a further important work variable is that of workload. according to maslach (1982), a large case load means that less time can be spent on each case, fewer services may be provided and there may be little or no follow-up. the helper may develop a quick and emotionally withdrawn method of dealing with her clients to try and avoid the emotional strain of dealing with so many people. this inadequate care provided may in turn induce feelings of inefficiency, failure and a lack of personal accomplishment and thus a large case load may represent a major source of burnout. fimian et al. (1991, p.433), found that "time and workload management" questions, consisting of items dealing with splp's perceptions of time at work, accounted for 32% of the stress variance. high scorers on this factor reported having "too much work to do, no time to get things done, little time to prepare, little time for personal priorities, and too much paperwork." one also needs to look at the effect of the type of clientele on the frequency of burnout. cherniss (1980), cites the example of helpers who have low functioning clientele. he argues that such persons are likely to believe that there is little they can do to improve their functioning leading to feelings of a poor sense of impact and efficacy. furthermore such professional caregivers are unlikely to be able to use their clinical skills to the fullest extent and may experience their roles as less consistent with their professional identities. tuomi (1993, p. 11), explains how quality care for the severely retarded person may just involve "doing something and maybe teaching the patient to cope with simple tasks..." these factors can have an effect on the experience of job stress and the coping reaction. these stressors within the work environment are likely to be further exacerbated by stressors emanating from broader societal factors. the broader sociocultiiral milieu according to handy (1988, p.354), "... the root causes of both stress and burnout are often far removed from the individual person or job and may be more appropriately conceptualised in jsocietal and organisational terms". an ecological perspective, where stress can be viewed as a result of an ι imbalance between environmental demands and resources, is possibly applicable to south african splp's and audiologists. becker and isaacs (1993, p.15), believe that "south africa is a country in transition. there are changes in the structure of society, in terms of social change and the elimination of apartheid, and in terms of massive moves from rural to urban settlement. concomitant with the promising aspects of societal change, there are, however, other aspects which place strain on the available health care services". a major source of strain is poverty which has a serious impact on individuals, families, and society in general. according to phiyega (1992), in south africa, poverty presents itself in political unrest due to stress and conflict, violence in families and communities, unemployment, lack of provisions and lack of facilities. hence, there are economic, political and social stressors operating in this country at the present time. an aspect of the inadequate provision of resources cited by beecham (1990), concerns the small number of speech and hearing therapy graduates trained in south africa which results, from aron's (1991) estimation of 1984 figures, in therapeutic services reaching only 138,000 persons from a possible client population of 3.5 million. in 1986, a survey conducted by the department of national health and population development found the following: in respect of so called "coloured" people, there was a shortage of 520 therapists. for asians the shortage was 139 while the shortage for the black population group was 3396. aron (1991), provided the following statistics: 223,200 persons with communication disorders receive attention in a period of a year, however, there are approximately three million who have communication disorders. clearly the ratio of therapists to patients requiring therapy is not improving with time. these figures lend support to becker and isaacs's (1993, p.24), contention that "in south africa the need to cater to increasing numbers of clients needing assessment and treatment is juxtaposed with the scarcity of trained professionals in the helping professions". from the above statistics it would seem likely that practising splp's and audiologists are being required to work with excessively large caseloads. secondly, therapists have to adapt their first world training to meet the need of particular social, educational and communicative problems within south africa's culturally diverse population. speech therapy and audiology can no longer safely slot into a tertiary level of intervention. splp's and audiologists now have to shift to intervention at a primary or secondary level, in communities where these needs are not being met. this may be placing an extra burden on the therapists who no longer confine themselves to giving therapy, but also have to tackle education of communities on relevant issues. steenekamp (1993, p.5), states that in the light of the economic situation "there is an increasing demand for the therapist's clinical skills, with a decreasing amount of time available for other demands, eg., research, tutoring, training of students, observation and so forth. increasing demands from other institutions are only adding to an increasing amount of stress." aron (1991), commented on the frustration endured, defending and interpreting the profession of speech pathology and audiology to official sources, university administrators and medical personnel. she states that this in fact may be a contributing factor in the burnout syndrome. clearly, south african splp's and audiologists appear to be subject to stresses at all levels of the social system, and to factors unique to the context of this country. it therefore appeared to be both relevant and timely to investigate the occurrence of burnout within members of this profession and to endeavour to relate frequency of burnout with factors occurring at the individual or personal level, within the work environment as well as the broader sociocultural milieu. although miller and potter (1982), conducted a national survey of burnout among splp's, criticism can be levelled against them because of their failure to use a standardised measure of burnout, such as the maslach burnout inventory (mbi). instead they merely asked respondents "how burned out they felt". in order to obtain more valid and reliable data on stress symptom levels in the profession, fimian et al., die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 marilyn swidler & eleanor ross (1991), developed the speech-language pathologist stress inventory (slpsi). more specifically, they constructed "... an instrument designed to measure job related sources and manifestations of stress experienced by speech-language pathologists in the schools..". these researchers attempted to adapt the mbi to speech pathologists. a possible weakness with the slpsi is that the authors averaged scores from individual subscales to yield a total burnout scale. koeske and koeske (1989, p. 142), advise against the use of this procedure for the following reason: "given the differential associations of the three subscales with one another and with other measures, researchers probably should avoid computing a composite "burnout" score and instead preserve the three subscale scores and relate them separately to other measures whenever appropriate". furthermore, this inventory was standardised on school speech-language pathologists only and may not necessarily tap possible stressors affecting speech-language pathologists in other job settings. the present study endeavoured to improve on the methodology utilised in these three overseas studies, and to extend their scope by incorporating standardised measures of burnout, co-worker support and supervisory support and by eliciting information on the broader sociocultural factors unique to south african splp's and audiologists. furthermore, it was hoped that the study would add to the theoretical knowledge of burnout and possibly provide practical suggestions for reducing the potentially deleterious effects on splp's and audiologists, their employing organisations and their clients. methodology aims the aims of the study were: (1) to investigate the frequency of burnout occurring among speech-language pathologists (splp's) and audiologists. (2) to determine the relationship between frequency of burnout and certain personal characteristics and work situation variables specific to splp's and audiologists. independent variables personal factors: gender work experience dependent variables burnout: emotional exhaustion depersonalization reduced personal accomplishment organisational factors: caseload place of work type of impairment treated severity of impairment treated adult/child working as splp/audiologist paperwork and administration language of client broader sociocultural factors perceived to contribute to the experience of stress, were analysed descriptively and were not included in the statistical analysis as no standardised scale is available to measure the phenomenon. subjects subject selection criteria a random probability sample of 500 names was drawn from the register of splps and audiologists in the republic of south africa registered with the s.a. medical and dental council as at june 1992. krejcie and morgan as cited by christenson (1980, p.299), suggest a sample size of 270 people to be an adequate representation of the total population of 852 people. description of subjects the sample comprised 500 therapists, 98% of whom were female and 2% were male. extent of work experience ranged from 1 year to 40 years with a mode of 11 years. with regard to place of work, 35% of respondents were employed in schools; 32% in private practice; 10% in hospitals; 7% in universities; and 16% in other settings. type of impairment treated was broken down as follows: language-learning disabilities 27%; language 20%; peripheral hearing loss 10.5%; central hearing loss 2%; articulation 8%; fluency 5.5%; traumatic brain injury 2%; mental retardation 3%; aphasia 1.5%; and other 2%. the vast majority of respondents, namely 73.5% worked with children only, while 4.5% worked solely with adults, and 22% provided services for both children and adults. in terms of the focus of intervention, only 7% of the sample were employed solely as audiologists; 44% worked only as speech pathologists; while 49% fulfilled the dual role of splp and audiologist. research procedure research design in order to investigate the aims of the study, a crosssectional, survey research design was employed which involved the use of a postal questionnaire. | description of the questionnaire a 9-page questionnaire (set out in appendix i) was constructed which could be completed in approximately 10-15 minutes. the research instrument was made available in both english and afrikaans translations as all respondents supposedly received their tertiary level education in either of these languages. a covering letter explained the purpose of the questionnaire, provided assurance of confidentiality, and clarified how and when to return the questionnaire. the questionnaire \ comprised the following sections: demographic information; open-ended questions on the respondent's perceptions of the influence of certain factors on stress and stress-coping strategies used by the respondents; subscales from moos and insel's work environment scale; and the maslach burnout inventory. these sections were included in order to provide information on the relationship between the frequency bf burnout being experienced, and biographical as well as work environment variables of the splp's and audiologists. each section is described separately as follows: the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) burnout: a smouldering problem 75 demographic information the aim of this section of the questionnaire was to s o l i c i t biographical information, namely respondent gender as well as data on factors such as job setting, caseload size, type of impairment, respondents subjective perceptions of severity of client's communication impairment and the amount of time spent on administration of paperwork. open ended questions this section of the questionnaire included items aimed to elicit respondents' perceptions of the possible influence of socio-economic and politico-cultural factors on the experience of work stress. also included were questions soliciting information on strategies used by respondents to cope with stress. finally, the respondents were asked whether they felt stress-management techniques should be taught to students as well as graduates. the work environment scale(wes) the wes is a 90-item, paper and pencil measure developed by moos and insel (1974), to assess the social climate of many types of work units, and focuses on relationships among employees, between employees and supervisors, and on the unit's basic organisational structure and functioning. the wes is composed of ten subscales that tap three underlying dimensions: relationship, personal growth, and system maintenance and change. from the relationship dimension two sub-scales were utilised in this research, namely: peer cohesion, and supervisor support. these sub-scales assess the extent to which employees are friendly to and supportive of one another, and the extent to which management is supportive of employees and encourages employees to be supportive of one another· (moos, 1986, p.l). from the personal growth or goal orientation dimension, one sub-scale was utilised, namely: work pressure. this sub-scale assesses the degree to which the press of work and time urgency dominates the work milieu (moos, 1986). j internal consistency calculated for each of the ten wes sub-scales using a sample of 1,045 employees, has been found to be within an acceptable range, i.e. 0.690.86 (moos, 1986, p.4). ! importantly, sub-scale inter-correlations indicate that the sub-scales measure distinct though somewhat related aspects of work environments. test-retest reliability has also been found to be acceptable. scales have also been reported to have satisfactory content and face validity as well as construct and criterion validity (moos, 1987). the maslach burnout inventory (mbi) the mbi, is a 22-item self-report scale developed by maslach and jackson (1981). responses involve the frequency of feelings towards each question, ranging from never (0) to "every day" (6). the instrument is composed of three sub-scales measuring the three areas that maslach and jackson believe best define the experience of burnout, namely: emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation, and lack of personal accomplishment. the instrument is labelled "human services survey" rather than "burnout" in order to avoid biasing respondents. the developers recommend that the inventory be presented as a survey of job-related attitudes without being connected to burnout and that anonymous responses be requested. in view of the fact that burnout is a multi-dimensional construct, the test does not yield an overall diagnosis of "burned-out" versus "non-burned out" but instead provides three separate, non-additive scores of emotional exhaustion, depersonalisation, and reduced personal accomplishment. maslach and jackson (1986) report that studies using the mbi have obtained high scores for internal consistency reliability, test-retest reliability, and convergent validity, and that the scale is free of social desirability bias. offerman (1985) states that the main limitations of the mbi is that it does not indicate the meaning of the score levels. the researcher needs to question what a "moderate" level of burnout means in terms of behaviour (offerman, 1985, p.421). nevertheless, the mbi has been in use for nearly twenty years and has been part of the methodology of hundreds of studies. research protocol pilot study the questionnaire was pre-tested on a small group of english and afrikaans speaking therapists not included in the final sample. distribution i) the target population comprised 500 registered splp's and audiologists. ii) in an attempt to increase the rate of returned completed questionnaires the researcher included stamped, addressed return envelopes and sent reminder cards requesting respondents to return questionnaires if they had not done so already. analysis of data initially the data was summarised using a univariate analysis procedure, which provided means where appropriate, standard deviations, modes, and ranges. following this, results were analysed to establish relationships between the independent and dependent variables using inferential statistics such as pearson productmoment correlations. this analysis was followed by a multivariate analysis of variance, using a general linear models procedure. finally bonferroni t-tests were used to further analyse the data, and establish differences between sub-groups. the open-ended questions were analysed descriptively by summarising and describing common themes expressed in the responses. results and discussion although 230 questionnaires were returned only 202 were used as the remainder arrived too late to be incorporated into the statistical analysis. the response rate was therefore 40.4% which moser and kalton (1971), die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafuiykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 marilyn swidler & eleanor ross regard as acceptable for mail surveys. missing data on certain measures being correlated reduced the sample size below 202. hence sample sizes ranged from 197202, depending on the measure being correlated. the question on the language spoken by the respondents' patients was not included in the statistical analysis, due to the ambiguity of the responses obtained. the first step in the data analysis involved obtaining the mean, mode, range and standard deviations of the variables. from the results it appeared that as a group splp's and audiologists were suffering from a moderate level of emotional exhaustion (i.e obtained a score between 17-26 on the mbi) although the range extended from a mild to high level of emotional exhaustion. the mean for depersonalisation was 3.8 indicating that as a group the respondents had a low tendency to depersonalise their patients (i.e. obtained a score between 0-6). these findings indicate that respondents have not, as a whole, developed a cynical opinion of their patients, expecting the worst from them and even disliking them. scores did, however, range from 0-22 with any score above 13 being considered as a high level of depersonalisation. the group indicated a low level of feelings of r e d u c e d personal a c c o m p l i s h m e n t (i.e.obtained a score between 0-31) with a mean of 10.23 and scores ranging from 0-28. these results suggest that splps were feeling competent and adequate in their jobs and their ability to relate to and treat their patients. these findings also underscore the fact that despite feeling emotionally exhausted, therapists can still derive a sense of accomplishment from the work and do not necessarily experience negative attitudes towards clients. these results are also consistent with oktay's (1992) findings that despite relatively high levels of emotional exhaustion experienced by hospital social workers who work with aids patients, they felt a substantially high level of personal accomplishment because of the meaningfulness of their work. table 1. burnout scores mean range std. dev mode low high emotional exhaustion 22.2 0 52 23 10.8 deperson. 3.8 0 22 0 4.3 personal accomplish 10.2 0 28 9 5.7 the relationships between the independent and dependent variables are discussed as follows: gender a negative relationship significant at the 0.05 level (f=6.41, p<.05) was found between depersonalisation and gender and no significant relationship was found between gender and the other two dependent variables of emotional exhaustion and reduced personal accomplishment. the research on gender and burnout is controversial with some researchers finding huge differences in the experiences of burnout between males and females and some finding very little difference. men are reportedly more susceptible to develop callous feelings about the people they work with and hence more likely to experience depersonalisation (maslach 1982;). the reasons offered by maslach (1982) are that men tend less towards close contact with people. however, there is no empirical data to support this reasoning. in addition, the present sample included only four males therefore making the generalisability of gender results more difficult. length of working experience a very significant negative relationship at the 0.01 level was found between the number of years the clinician has been working and depersonalisation, although this was not supported by the multivariate analysis (f=0.92, p>.05) or the bonferroni t-test. research has shown that burnout is lower among older and more experienced workers (maslach 1982; van der ploeg et al.,1990; ross & altmaier, 1989). it seems that it is not just a case of work experience but also the maturity and stability that accompanies increasing age. edelwich and brodsky (1980) suggested that newly qualified professionals are prone to experience burnout when their initial energy and ideals are eroded by the real difficulties and limitations of their profession. this could be especially true for south african trained speech therapists who are taught western techniques during their training which may no longer be appropriate in a multi-cultural society such as south africa and when the ideal clinic situation during their university training is no longer applicable in the real job context. of related interest is that a highly significant relationship at the 0.01 level of confidence was found between the number of years worked and the amount of paperwork completed. pines and maslach (1978) as cited by savicki and cooley (1987) offer an explanation for the above, suggesting that as workers remain in the field longer, they tend to become less involved in direct therapy work and more involved in administration and administrative duties which may be impacting on burnout in a unique way. • ι place of work ι ι a significant negative relationship was found at the 0.05 level (f=5.43, p<.005) between place of work and emotional exhaustion, and a very significant relationship at the 0.01 level (f=2.85, p<.05) existed between place of work and depersonalisation. t-tests revealed that therapists working in a hospital setting tended to experience more emotional exhaustion than therapists working in private practice, and therapists working in institutions experienced more emotional exhaustion than those working in private practise. these findings suggest that the clinical setting and the work environment play a role in the type and severity of stress experienced by professionals. / caseload also of interest is the significant correlations at the 0.005 level between place of work and caseload and at the 0.05 level between place of work and the severity of the client treated..these relationships possibly offer an the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) burnout: a smouldering problem 77 explanation for the relationship between place of work and emotional exhaustion. furthermore a significant r e l a t i o n s h i p existed b e t w e e n place of work and depersonalisation (f=2.85, p<.05). hospital therapists tended to experience more depersonalisation than either school therapists or private practitioners. a very significant relationship at the 0.01 level ( f=5.43, p<.001) was found between case load and emotional exhaustion. according to maslach (1982, p.38), a large case load may lead to a situation of "too many people and too little time to adequately serve their needs— a situation ripe for burnout." furthermore larger caseloads mean that more energy is depleted in order to perform satisfactorily. type of impairment a significant relationship at the 0.05 level ( f=2.50, p<.05) exists between the type of defect treated and depersonalisation. t-tests revealed that treating peripheral hearing disorders tended to be associated with greater depersonalisation of patients than treating both a language and a language/ learning disability. this could possibly be attributed to the fact that treating a peripheral hearing disability usually involves three or four sessions or perhaps even one diagnostic session, therefore the therapist is not afforded the opportunity to establish a relationship with her patients. paperwork a very significant relationship was found at the 0.001 level ( f=2.67, p<.05) between the amount of paperwork completed in a week and emotional exhaustion. according to maslach (1982), administrative tasks and paperwork can interfere with the therapists' direct work with the patient as it could take up therapy time. in fact many of the therapists reported in'the open-ended questions, that paperwork often needed to be done after hours as there was usually not enough time during the workday to complete all the necessary {administration. hence long work hours may be leading to feelings of emotional exhaustion. ι co-worker support j a very significant relationship exists at the 0.001 level between emotional exhaustion and co-worker support. this relationship was, however, not in the expected direction. statistical analysis of the data, revealed the presence of a few outlying values which could have grossly distorted the data, resulting in a spurious rather than a meaningful or substantive correlation. work pressure work pressure and emotional exhaustion were highly correlated with each other and a very significant relationship at the 0.001 level (f=5.76, p<.001) existed between them. cherniss (1980, p.44) maintains that "stress occurs when there is a perceived imbalance between resources and demands. demands can be external (for instance formal job requirements) or internal (for example, personal goals, needs, and moral values)." since it appears that external demands such as caseload, impairment treated, and lack of social support did not seem to be correlated significantly or were minimally correlated with the burnout phenomenon, it would seem that these demands are possibly internal as reported by some of the private practitioners in their open-ended questions. it is possible that practitioners were not perceiving their work demands realistically and were constantly plagued by a sense of urgency and lack of time to complete their worki descriptive analysis of open-ended responses question 1 work stressors common themes emerged in the responses of private practitioners and those working at schools and hospitals. rules, regulations and bureaucracy, writing of detailed reports as well as inspections were considered as stressful factors at work. those working in schools needed to use groupwork as a therapeutic technique due to time constraints and a situation of too many pupils and too few therapists and they tended to find groupwork stressful and emotionally draining. they claimed that this method was associated with poor progress of pupils due to insufficient treatment time. poor cooperation between teachers, parents and therapists was a problem cited by many respondents. private therapists complained of poor cooperation of parents with home programmes. they were pressurised to obtain results in a certain period of time and often parents and teachers had unrealistic expectations of therapy results. a few therapists found it stressful that patients who travel from far expect results within periods as short as in three sessions. furthermore, private therapists were expected to make an immediate diagnosis, leaving them no time to test thoroughly and arrive at a carefully considered diagnosis. working in isolation was found to be extremely difficult as one cannot confirm results and diagnosis with another therapist. financial handling of a private practice and collection of fees was reported to be highly stressful. lack of training in certain areas of treatment was also found to be stressful for some therapists. inadequate test materials and inappropriate standardised tests due to cultural and language biases increased the stresses of therapy. many therapists felt that the profession has not seemed to have established itself. some respondents highlighted the fact that one continually needed to promote the profession as there was insufficient knowledge about speech-language pathology and audiology and the contribution it can make. time restraints and time pressure were found to be stressful by the majority of therapists irrespective of their job context. time management was not confined to therapy but included school visits, speaking to parents, writing reports and programmes. there appeared to be little time for research and reading to keep pace with the unending amount of new information. therapists often needed to work after hours to keep up to date with administration and paperwork. therapists reported sometimes feeling ill-equipped to deal with emotional support and counselling during the session and the emotional difficulties involved in working with severely impaired patients who show little improvement. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 marilyn swidler & eleanor ross question 2 -societal stressors handy (1988) cautions that societal and organisational causes of stress cannot be ignored simply because there are no easy solutions to solving these problems. figure 1 indicates that 30% of the respondents felt that broader societal factors did not contribute to their experience of stress. however, among the 70% of respondents who perceived themselves to be affected by socioeconomic and politico-cultural factors, common themes were expressed. figure 1. affected by sociocultural factors. therapists found it stressful that children from poor economic backgrounds seem to show little progress in therapy. one of the most important demands confronting human service staff is the expectation of competence and many caregivers are motivated to achieve a sense of efficacy or psychological success in their work (cherniss, 1980). lack of progress in therapy might therefore be stressful for therapists. the respondents attributed this occurrence to poor parental education levels, the fact that patients often do not attend therapy, follow home programmes or advice given and do not understand the implications of the disorder. in addition to this, caregivers often have to work and there is little stimulation in the home. another related issue was that many patients cannot afford to come regularly to therapy. furthermore, new medical aid cutbacks have frequently required reducing the number of therapy sessions and thus therapists feel that because of rising costs they are more accountable to achieve rapid results. many therapists were also distressed at the prospect of not being able to treat patients who could not afford clinic fees. several respondents reported that for those therapists not in private practise, the number of posts available has been reduced resulting in large workloads and little job satisfaction. salaries have been cut or are not being increased making it difficult to meet the rising cost of living. political unrest results in therapists fearing for their safety and the security of their families. a few therapists found that travelling to work was stressful as they feared rioting or being attacked. the above are examples of physical work stressors which can potentially influence workers' perception of job comfort. there appeared to be uncertainty as to the future of the profession in a third world country where primary needs are not being met. there is often a lack of reliable transport to and from the townships and violence and stayaways seriously disrupt provision of services. the poor economy and uncertain politics— including political differences among co-workers, impacts on enthusiasm and ease of working conditions. from a sociocultural point of view therapists remarked on the difficulty of working with patients from different cultures and different languages. person-role conflict can contribute to burnout (cherniss, 1980). many therapists indicated that they felt unprepared to work with other communities because they lack the necessary knowledge and skills. the implications of these findings are that if the splp's and audiologists are not adequately trained for new role demands, the consequences are likely to be high levels of person-role conflict, j o b stress, dissatisfaction and b u r n o u t (cherniss, 1980). some therapists commented on the language barrier between teachers and therapists and the difficulty in advising teachers on how to handle second language learners. on the other hand those therapists serving only white south africans were concerned that they were not addressing the needs of the larger community. furthermore, many of the children treated live with their grandparents and subsequently it was difficult to obtain medical and genetic histories. finally, parents may not bring their children for an assessment until they are older and the problem has become compounded and multifaceted and in some cases resulting in poor prognoses. question 3 -stress-coping strategies "coping refers to changing cognitive and behavioral efforts used to manage internal or external demands perceived as exceeding the resources of the individual" ( folkman, lazarus, gruen & de longis, 1986, p.23). strategies used to cope with stress varied and appear to be highly individualistic. common techniques utilised involved regular exercise, time away from jwork, religious beliefs as well as careful planning ancl organisation of work schedules and demands. some negative strategies such as smoking, overeating and drinking coffee were also reported. i question 4 stress-management course during training the majority of respondents, namely, 62% of the respondents felt that a stress management course should be incorporated into the training curricula of speechlanguage and hearing pathologists. others felt that the course was too full and that to include another subject would only serve to increase the stress experienced by students. furthermore, it was felt that if time was available it should rather be utilised for areas that were not taught in detail. others felt that such a course would be useless without specific job experience, and that different work contexts cause different forms of stress. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) burnout: a smouldering problem 79 wilder and plutchik cited in paine (1982, p.115) agree, believing that " just giving knowledge to professional trainees about stress and the phenomenon of burnout during training will probably have few long term effects". figure 2. stress management course for students. question 5 -stress-management course for qualified therapists ί a high proportion of respondents, namely 80%, felt that a stress-managementjcourse should be made available for qualified speech-language and hearing pathologists. it was, however, specified by many respondents that this course should not be run by fellow speech-language therapists and audiologists as the subject matter was regarded as too personal. among those who felt the course was unnecessary, reasons given were that percentage figure 3. stress management for qualified therapists stress management is a personal choice and is too varied to apply to everyone. conclusions the data suggest that although moderate levels of burnout characterised the sample on average, certain job characteristics tended to make splp's more susceptible to this experience. furthermore, despite the fact that many respondents were subject to difficult work conditions which tended to give rise to feelings of emotional exhaustion, the majority of therapists reported deriving a sense of accomplishment from their work and relatively low levels of depersonalisation. it is important to note that this study provided correlational data and thus no conclusions about causal relations among socio-demographic factors, work setting variables and burnout can be drawn. nevertheless, the findings from the study do have implications for clinical practice and job design; research; and the training and continuing education of speech-language pathologists and audiologists. clinical practice and job design in view of the fact that certain job settings such as hospitals, appear to contain more potential for job stress than other contexts, it is recommended that employers of splps and audiologists pay attention to the structure and design of the job. for example, support meetings, informal case discussions, supervision and buddy or mentorship systems are alternate strategies that could possibly be built into work structures. these support mechanisms would appear to have particular relevance to newly qualified therapists with limited work experience who were shown to be particularly prone to experience high rates of depersonalisation. it is also suggested that care be taken to control the number of difficult cases allocated to therapists and the supervision of those cases. moreover, in view of the significantly high correlation between depersonalisation and treatment of peripheral hearing disorders, it is recommended that— where practically feasible— supervisors endeavour to allocate a wide range of patients with different types and degrees of communication impairment to the therapists in their employment. in addition, the complaint expressed by a large number of respondents regarding excessive paperwork, suggests the need for employers and clinicians to brainstorm ways of possibly streamlining routine administrative tasks, particularly in large bureaucratic organisations. research it is recommended that future studies incorporate personality variables into the research design and analysis as these factors may predispose some splps and audiologists to be susceptible to burnout. it is possible that certain personality characteristics are looked for in the selection procedures for training in the profession orto quote one respondent"perhaps the feeling of work pressure instilled in us during our training, is an image that we carry with us into the "real' world, thus affecting our perceptions of work demands." die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 m a r i l y n s w i d l e r & e l e a n o r r o s s a n o t h e r i m p o r t a n t a s p e c t w h i c h w a s o m i t t e d from the s t a t i s t i c a l a n a l y s i s i n the p r e s e n t s t u d y a n d w h i c h n e e d s to b e r e s e a r c h e d , w a s the effect o f l a n g u a g e s spok e n b y the c l i e n t s o f s p l p s a n d a u d i o l o g i s t s on b u r n out. f u r t h e r m o r e a s u b j e c t i v e m e a s u r e of client c o m m u n i c a t i o n i m p a i r m e n t w a s u t i l i s e d i n this study. it is s u g g e s t e d t h a t future r e s e a r c h e n d e a v o u r s to i n c o r p o rate m o r e o b j e c t i v e m e a s u r e s o f t h i s v a r i a b l e . a further fruitfu-l area for future r e s e a r c h is the different c o p i n g styles adopted b y speech-language pathologists a n d a u d i o l o g i s t s as t h e y c a n influence h o w indiv i d u a l s r e s p o n d to stress. t h e p r e s e n t s t u d y focused o n g e n e r a l w o r k s t r e s s e s e x p e r i e n c e d b y a n a t i o n a l s a m p l e of t h e r a p i s t s w o r k i n g i n v a r i o u s j o b s e t t i n g s . f u t u r e r e s e a r c h n e e d s to focus on specific s t r e s s e s a s s o c i a t e d w i t h specific j o b contexts s u c h as a i d s p a t i e n t s , n e u r o l o g i c a l disorders and so forth. a further l i m i t a t i o n of t h e s t u d y c e n t r e d on the use o f t h e w o r k e n v i r o n m e n t scale w h i c h m e a s u r e s perc e p t i o n s o f r e s p o n d e n t s r e g a r d i n g their w o r k milieu. p e r c e p t i o n s m a y h a v e b e e n u n i n t e n t i o n a l l y distorted or subjects m i g h t h a v e provided socially desirable answers. for t h i s r e a s o n future r e s e a r c h c o u l d p r o b a b l y i n c o r p o rate m o r e o b j e c t i v e i n d i c e s o f the j o b b y focussing on the a c t u a l n u m b e r o f c a s e s s e e n e a c h w e e k , a n d the actual t i m e d e v o t e d to p a p e r w o r k . t r a i n i n g a n d c o n t i n u i n g e d u c a t i o n t h e fact t h a t 6 2 % of the r e s p o n d e n t s w e r e in favour of a s t r e s s m a n a g e m e n t c o u r s e b e i n g i n c o r p o r a t e d into the u n d e r g r a d u a t e t r a i n i n g c u r r i c u l u m and 8 0 % felt t h a t a c o u r s e s h o u l d b e m a d e a v a i l a b l e to qualified s p l p s a n d a u d i o l o g i s t s , u n d e r l i n e s t h e n e e d to a d d r e s s this g a p i n professional e d u c a t i o n . in c o n c l u s i o n , it is h o p e d t h a t t h i s s t u d y offers i m p o r t a n t g u i d e l i n e s for future r e s e a r c h a n d the d e v e l o p m e n t o f i n t e r v e n t i o n p r o g r a m m e s a n d for future r e s e a r c h a n d e d u c a t i o n . if s p e e c h p a t h o l o g i s t s and audiologists are to s u r v i v e the d e m a n d s o f the 1990's, a n d are to c o n t i n u e to b e influential a n d effective profess i o n a l s , t h e y w i l l n e e d to identify a n d m a n a g e stressr e l a t e d p r o b l e m s before the i m p a c t o f t h e s e p r o b l e m s b e c o m e s e x c e s s i v e a n d t h e i r s o l u t i o n difficult. a c k n o w l e d g e m e n t s financial a s s i s t a n c e received from the s.a. speech-lang u a g e a n d h e a r i n g a s s o c i a t i o n t o w a r d s the costs of the study, is a c k n o w l e d g e d w i t h a p p r e c i a t i o n . r e f e r e n c e s aron, m.l. perspectives. (1991) the south african journal of communication disorders38, 3-11. barling, j. (1986). some consequences of paid employment. in j. barling, c. fullager & s.d. bluen (eds.). behavior in organisations: south african perspectives. 2nd ed. johannesburg: university of the witwatersrand. becker, l. & isaacs, g. (1993), brief dynamic psycho-therapy: an exploration of attitudes and practice among a group of clinicians in cape town, south africa.some implications for training. social work, 29(1), 14-31. beecham, r. (1990). training and context: perceptions of south african speech therapists and audiologists. honours research project, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. bernstein, c. (1992). supervisor social support as a moderator of stress-strain relationships. m.a. psychology dissertation. johannesburg: university of the witwatersrand. cherniss, c. (1980). staff burnout. job stress in the human services. california: sage publications. christensen, l.b. (1980). experimental methodology. allyn and bacon, inc: boston. cobb, s. (1976). social support as a moderator of life stress. psychosomatic medicine, 38(5), 300-314 cooper, c.l. & davidson, m.j. (1991). the stress survivors: experiences of successful personalities. london:grafton disability in the republic of south africa. (1987). speech impairment, vol 10, commissioned by the department of health, for the year of the disabled. edelwich, j. & brodsky, a. (1980). burnout: stages of disillusionment in the helping profession. human sciences press:new york. fimian, m.j. lieberman, r.j. & fastenau, p.s. (1991). development and validation of an instrument to measure occupational stress in speech-language pathologists. journal of speech and hearing research , 34, 439-446. folkman, s., lazarus, r.s., gruen, r.j. & de longis, a. (1986). appraisal, coping, health status, and psychological symptoms. journal of personality and social psychology, 50(3), 571-579. freudenberger, h.j. (1974). staff burnout. journal of social issues, 30, 159-165. handy, j.a. (1988). theoretical and methodological problems within occupational stress and burnout research. human relations, 41(5), 351-369. kersner, m. & stone, j. (1991). stress in the national health service: a survey of three therapy groups. stress news, 2 (4), 2-9. kobasa, s. (1979). stressful life events, personality, ill-health: an inquiry into hardiness. journal of social personality, 37, 1-11. koeske, g.f. & koeske, r.d.(1989). construct validity of the maslach burnout inventory: a critical review and reconceptualisation. journal of applied behavioral science, 25(2), 131-143 mac farlane, w.r. (1990). social support as a moderator of the burnout/organisational behavior relationship. honours dissertation, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. maslach, c. (1982). burnout the cost of caring. prenticehall press: new york. maslach, c. & jackson, s. (1981). maslach burnout inventory manual. research ed. california: consulting psychologists press. ' maslach, c. & jackson, s. (1986). maslach burnout inventory manual. 2nd edition. california psychologists press. miller, m.m. & potter, r.e. (1982). professional burnout among speech language pathologists. asha 1982, 177-181 moos, r.h. (1987). the social climate scales: a userls guide* california: consulting psychologists press. moos, r.h. & insel, p.n. (1974). work environment scale. california: consulting psychologists press. moos, r.h. (1986). work environment scale manual. california: consulting psychologists press. ' moser, c. & kalton, g. (1971). survey methods in social investigation. 2nd ed. london: heinemann. offermann, l.r. (1985). maslach burnout inventory. in d. keyser & r. sweetland (edsjifeslcriiigues. vol iii, u.s.a.: test corporation of america. oktay, j. (1992). burnout in hospital social workers who work with aids patients. social work, 37(5), 432-439. paine, w.s. (1982). job stress and burnout: research, theory and intervention perspectives. beverly hills:' sage publications. phiyega, m.v. (1992). community development-the south african welfare challenge: aresppnse by the south african national council for child and family welfare. the social work practitioner-researcher, 3, 3-7. ratcliff, n. (1988). stress and burnout in the helping professions. social casework, 69(3), 147-154. ross, r.r., altmaier, e.m., & russell, d.w. (1989). job stress, social support, and burnout among counseling center staff. journal of counselling psychology, 36(4), 464-470. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) burnout: a smouldering problem savicki, v. & cooley, e. (1987). the relationship of work environment and client contact to burnout in mental health professionals. journal of counselling and development. 65, 249-252. steenekamp, a.m. (1993) editorial. communiphon, no.305, 3-6. 81 tuomi, s.p. (1993). quality assurance in speechlanguage pathology and audiology. the south african experience. communiphon, no. 305, 10-13. van der ploeg, h.m, van leeuwen, j.j, & kwee, m.g.t. (1990). burnout among dutch psychotherapists. psychological reports, 67, 107-112 appendix i dear speech pathologist/ audiologist as a requirement for my degree, i am conducting a study of occupational stress among south african speechlanguage pathologists and audiologists, and the specific work situations which clinicians are currently experiencing. i believe that therapists working in this country are having to deal not only with the day-to -day stresses of all helping professionals but also with the unique economic and socio-political conditions prevalent in our society. your participation in this study will add to our knowledge and understanding of work stress within our profession. furthermore, depending on the level of stress found to be experienced, intervention strategies may need to be recommended both at a training and occupational level. it would therefore be highly appreciated if each one of you could take the time to fill out the enclosed questionnaire thereby helping to ensure that the findings of the study are representative of the profession of speech-language pathology and audiology. please would you assist by answering each item as openly and honestly as possible, and when completed, sealing the questionnaire in the envelope provided, and posting it back to me by the 24th of april at the latest. there are no wrong or right answers. rather, what i am looking for, is the response that is most accurate for you. please note that your responses will be kept strictly confidential and you will remain anonymous as your name is not required anywhere on this questionnaire. supplied with this questionnaire is a postcard with your name written on it. kindly return the postcard separately, informing me that the questionnaire has been returned and no follow-up questionnaires will be sent to you. if you are interested in the results or have any questions, i will be only too willing to give you feedback at a mutually acceptable time. i can be contacted at telephone number: 453-7025. thank you again for your time and cooperation. yours sincerely marilyn swidler demographic data 1. your sex: 1) female 1 2) male 2. how many years have you worked in the profession since graduation? 3. please check the main work setting in which you are currently employed. ι a) hospital-clinic b) private clinic c) school d) insitution e) university f) private practise g) other 4. as of this time, my client caseload per week is: a) 1-15 b) 16-30 c) 31-45 d) 46-60 e) 60 or more 5. most of my clients have defects of: (select only one). a) articulation b) language c) language/learning difficulties d) aphasia e) traumatic brain injury f) degenerative neurological disorders g) dysphagia h) fluency i) peripheral hearing disorders j) central hearing disorders k) mental retardation 1) other (specify) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 marilyn swidler & eleanor ross 6. are the majority of your caseload: a) mildly impaired b) moderately impaired c) severely impaired 3. what strategies do you use to cope with stress? open-ended questions the following statements are about the place in which you work. the statements are intended to apply to all work environments. however, some words may not be quite suitable for your work environment, for example the term "supervisor" can also refer to a manager, department head, or the person or persons to whom an employee reports. you are to decide which statements are true of your work environment and which are false. if you think the statement is true or mostly true of your work environment, make an x in the corresponding box in| the column labelled true. if you think the statement |is false or mostly false of your work environment, make an x in the corresponding box in the column labelled false. if,a statement is not applicable to your work environment please mark the n/a column. please be sure to answer every statement. section a 1. people go out of their way ' to help a new employee feel comfortable. 2. the atmosphere is somewhat impersonal. 3. people take a personal interest in each other. true false n/a the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) burnout: a smouldering problem 83 4. employees rarely do things together after work. 5. people are generally frank about how they feel. 6. employees often eat lunch together. 7. employees who differ greatly from the others in the organisation don't get on well. 8. employees often talk to each other about their personal problems. section β 1. supervisors really stand up for their people. 2. employees discuss their personal problems with supervisors. 3. supervisors expect far too much from their employees. 4. employees generally feel free to ask for a raise. 5. supervisors often criticise employees over, minor things. 6. supervisors usually give full credit to ideas contributed employees. [ j 7. supervisors tend to ! discourage criticisms j from employees. j 8. supervisors tend to talk down to employees. 9. supervisors usually compliment an employee when he does something well. section c 1. people often have to work overtime to get their work done. 2. people cannot afford to relax. 3. you can take it easy i and still get your work done. true false n/a true false n/a 4. there is no time pressure. 5. it is very hard to keep up with your workload. 6. nobody works too hard. 7. there are always deadlines to be met. 8. there is a constant pressure to keep working. 9. there always seems to be an urgency about everything. human services survey the purpose of this section is to discover how speechlanguage pathologists and audiologists as human service or helping professionals, view their jobs and the people with whom they work closely. the term "client" refers to the people for whom you provide service, care, treatment or instruction — even though you may use another term in your work, eg., patient, student, employee, etc. there are 22 statements of job-related feelings. please read each statement carefully and decide if you ever feel this way about your job. if you have never had this feeling, write a "0" (zero) before the statement. if you have had this feeling, indicate how often you feel it by writing the number (from 1 to 6) that best describes how frequently you feel this way. how often true false n/a 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 never a few once a a few once a a few every times month times week times day a year or less a a or less month week how often 0 6 statements 1. i feel emotionally drained from my work. 2. i feel used up at the end of the workday. 3. i feel fatigued when i get up in the morning and have to face another day on the job. 4. i can easily understand how my clients feel about things. 5. i feel i treat some clients as if they were impersonal objects 6. working with people all day is really a strain for me. 7. i deal very effectively with problems of my clients. 8. i feel burned out from my work. 9. i feel i'm positively influencing other people's lives through my work. 10. i've become more callous toward people since i took this job. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 84 11. i worry that this job is hardening me emotionally. 12. i feel very energetic. 13. i feel frustrated by my job. 14. i feel i'm working too hard on my job. 15. i don't really care what happens to some clients. 16. working with people directly puts too much stress on me. 17. i can easily create a relaxed atmosphere with my clients. marilyn swidler & eleanor ross 18. i feel exhilarated after working closely with my clients. 19. i have accomplished many worthwhile things in this job. 20. i feel like i'm at the end of my rope. 21. in my work, i deal with emotional problems very calmly. 22. i feel clients blame me for some of their problems. a new australian phonological profile for complex phonological disorders developed and trialled in western australia initially designed for use with profoundly deaf children, this profile can deal with the analysis of the most complex phonological cases, including those in hearing children. unique profile format that provides information about • vowels .consonants .clusters ' • • " s s s s s & s ^ • processes tfte kit contains •profile sheets , r * stimulus picttjrfs •recording sheets -manual * c a s e l ^ s u g g e s l l o n s f o r remediation) iris vardi tel/fax 6 1 9 3 6 7 1 5 7 8 marie kormendy tel 6 1 9 3 2 8 9 4 0 6 6 1 9 3 8 3 8 3 4 0 fax 6 1 9 3 8 7 7 0 9 5 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language teaching.html language teaching is no panacea: a theoretical perspective and critical evaluation of language in education within the south african context heila jordaan department of speech pathology and audiology, school of human and community development, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg correspondence to: h jordaan (heila.jordaan@wits.ac.za) abstract language competence is both the means and the end to educational achievement, and multilingualism in particular has important cognitive, academic and societal advantages. the linguistic diversity in south africa creates an ideal context to provide learners with the educational opportunities that promote high levels of linguistic proficiency in their home and additional languages. unfortunately, the education system has not delivered on the constitutional imperatives of promoting multilingualism. english continues to dominate as the preferred language of teaching and learning, at the expense and marginalisation of the african languages. this is regarded by many researchers as the primary reason for the disturbingly low numeracy and literacy achievement levels of the majority of south african schoolchildren. however, the effects of language-in-education practices on academic achievement are not straightforward. this paper analyses recent research pertaining to the effects of language-in-education practices and argues that the critical role of educational linguistics is neglected in the south african education system. this affects the quality of teaching irrespective of the language of instruction and has a significant impact on the achievement of our children. the purpose of this paper is to present a critical theoretical perspective on language in education in order to influence policy and practice. an additional aim is to promote the role of speech-language therapists (slts) in education, since these professionals are well positioned to work in collaboration with educators to enhance language learning in mainstream/ordinary classrooms. however, slts also need to be well informed about the challenges in education and the theory underlying language-in-education practices. keywords: language-in-education; the role of the slt the current south african education system is in crisis and confronted with serious challenges (fleisch, 2008; webb, lafon & pare, 2010). a number of recent large-scale national and international research projects have unequivocally shown that the majority of south african children have very low literacy and numeracy levels (department of education, 2005; reddy, 2005; moloi & strauss, 2005; taylor & yu, 2008; mtshali & smillie, 2011). although the problems in education are exacerbated by conditions of poverty and poor teaching quality (fleisch, 2008; reddy, kanjee, diedericks & winnaar, 2006), many educational researchers propose that the main reason for the poor performance of south african schoolchildren is that the majority are learning in english as an additional language (heugh, 2009; brock-utne & skattum, 2009; alexander, 2005). the term ‘additional language’ as opposed to ‘second language’ is used, since many south african children are exposed to more than one language in the home and community in addition to and often before learning english. while it is true that english is dominant in south africa, both in the public domain and in education, the effects of language-in-education practices are not straightforward. there is a complex interaction of factors causing poor achievement, and research on language-in-education practices has yielded inconsistent results. the central argument of this paper is that one of the most important reasons for the poor achievement of south african learners is that the pivotal role of language in education is neglected in the curriculum and in teacher-training programmes, resulting in limited language awareness, and consequently inadequate teaching methods that lead to language difficulties across all curriculum areas. this is a problem irrespective of whether english or one of the african languages serves as the medium of instruction, or is the subject of study. this argument is based on a number of theoretical constructs. the first is the distinction between social and academic language (cummins, 2008), which is not acknowledged in the outcomes-based education (obe) assessment standards. the development of the academic language register is one of the primary goals of education, since it underlies literacy, mathematics, and meaningful engagement with the subject matter at all stages of education (scarcella, 2011). it is not acquired as naturally as the social-communicative functions of language and develops through formal instruction at all stages of the education process (cummins, 2008; scarcella, 2011). the second is social constructivism as a theory of language learning. although obe claims to be constructivist (heugh, 2009), it is essentially positivist in that it focuses on performance and outcomes, and not on the processes essential for language learning (balfour, 2007; reagan, 2009). the third is content and language integrated learning (clil) (coyle, 2008; sherris, 2008), which is highly effective in contexts where learners are required to master both the subject matter and language of learning, as is the case for the majority of south african children. research on children instructed in english as an additional language in contrast to the generally positive findings on the achievement of african children educated in their home language and/or in bilingual programmes (heugh, 2009; obondo, 2008), the language problems experienced by children who are instructed in a second or additional language have been extensively researched both internationally and in south africa, and a growing number of educational researchers attribute underachievement to learning in a second or additional language (alexander, 2005; brock-utne & skattum, 2009; pluddeman, vuyokazi & ncedo, 2010, in webb et al., 2010). these authors suggest that the choice of english by a large proportion of african-language-speaking parents is undermining the academic achievement of the children they are seeking to empower through education. in most research studies there is almost complete agreement that underachievement is linked to instruction and assessment in english. these studies, which all suggest an achievement gap between english first-language (efl) and additional-language learners (eal), are discussed below. referring to the third international mathematics and science study (timss) on 900 grade 8 learners, in which south africa obtained an average score of 264 for mathematics compared with the international average of 467, howie (2005a) states that fluency in english was the most significant determinant in learning science and mathematics. reddy et al. (2006) report that in the 2003 timms test, children who ‘always’ spoke the language of the test at home scored an average of 349, while those who ‘never’ spoke the language of the test at home scored only 192. the western cape grade 6 assessment study (wced, 2004) showed that children who spoke english as a first language (l1) had a mean score of 70% on the literacy test, while isixhosa l1 speakers obtained only 37%. only 1.6% of isixhosa speakers performed at official grade level. broom (2004) investigated the reading achievement of grade 3 learners in 20 urban primary schools in gauteng, and found that the average score on the reading test for eal learners in township schools was 31.8%, as opposed to the 87.8% achieved by efl learners. the performance of efl learners was consistently higher than that of eal learners, even in the same urban schools, but the performance of eal learners in the urban schools was better than that of their peers in the township schools. a number of studies have also investigated the language proficiency, as opposed to educational outcomes, of children learning in english. a study by jooste (2003) on grade 5 children in a number of upper and lower socio-economic status (ses) schools in cape town, showed that by grade 5, eal learners were still performing significantly below their efl peers on measures of reading comprehension. van rooyen and jordaan (2009) assessed 464 grade 8 12 learners in an ex-model-c high school on a measure of complex sentence comprehension. although the results of the study indicated that the majority of learners achieved within the average range, and there were no statistically significant differences between efl and eal learners, the eal participants tended to score, on average, one scaled score below the efl participants, suggesting that they had not quite reached the proficiency levels of the first-language participants. this means that despite a substantial period of educational exposure to english throughout the primary and high school grades, parity with monolingual peers is not always achieved, even on oral tasks. one would therefore expect greater differences on reading or written tasks, which demand higher levels of language processing. in addition, the language comprehension scores were positively correlated with the most recent school report mark and the most recent english mark, confirming that oral language proficiency underlies academic achievement (cummins, 2008). webb et al. (2010, p. 279) claim that the choice of english as the medium of instruction by learners in the ex-model-c schools is not as problematic as in rural and township schools, because in the former context the learners have ‘reasonably adequate’ english proficiency. however, this may be an assumption based on the research evidence showing that middle-class children of all races in this system perform as well as those in international contexts on measures of numeracy and literacy and go on to obtain a university entrance exemption and education (fleisch, 2008). it should be noted that the research referred to by fleisch (2008) does not consider the differences between efl and eal learners in these schools and some of the reported difficulties, particularly related to language, experienced by teachers and learners in ex-model-c contexts (du plessis & naude, 2003; du plessis & louw, 2008; o’connor & geiger, 2009). for example, meier (2005, p. 171) found that teachers in this context experienced increased workloads in accommodating learners from diverse backgrounds since they had to adapt their teaching methods by teaching at different levels, sometimes ‘lowering standards’, and they had not been prepared to deal with multilingualism in the classroom. from the eal learners’ perspective, the complexity of the english used by efl teachers demands high levels of auditory processing and short-term memory and can lead to attention problems (brice & brice, 2000). if these children experience academic problems as a result of learning in english, this leads to lowered self-esteem and a lack of confidence (o’connor & geiger, 2009). in a study on 80 foundation-phase teachers in ex-model-c schools in the cape town metropolitan area, o’connor and geiger (2009) found that teachers expressed difficulties with discipline as a result of comprehension problems in eal learners, and had limited success in collaborating with parents, because of social circumstances such as extended working hours, transport problems and financial difficulties. parents’ limited english proficiency and low literacy levels were also identified as problems since they could not assist with homework. teachers found it difficult to teach at different linguistic levels when they had both eal and efl children in their classes, and experienced time pressures when they had to pre-teach the vocabulary and concepts required in a particular lesson. efl teachers were frustrated by not being able to speak the home languages of the children in their classes and did not always understand the influence of the home language on the learning of english. teachers expressed a need for support such as assistants who speak african languages, language-enrichment teachers, and language-teaching resources. they also felt that their training was not adequate in either multilingualism or teaching practice to equip them for the task of educating learners from diverse linguistic backgrounds. it is therefore apparent that one cannot assume that eal learners in ex-model-c schools are necessarily achieving the english proficiency they need to perform to their full potential academically. furthermore it has become evident in studies of university students (pienaar, 2009) that language problems are perpetuated at this level, and there is growing concern among academics that many students, regardless of educational background and whether they are l1 or second-language (l2) speakers of the language of instruction at the university, enter the tertiary level with weak language and literacy skills and are ill-equipped to deal with the demands of academic language in the various disciplines. this is particularly reflected in their writing skills. questions can therefore be raised regarding the language-learning processes in all schools and whether children attain the language proficiency and consequent literacy skills required for the increasing conceptual demands of the curriculum in the higher grades and beyond. fleisch (2008, p. 98) points out that although the research shows a relationship between achievement and instruction in english, the studies often do not provide insights into the ‘generative mechanisms, the underlying reasons or causes that link children’s experiences with language at school and their failure to become proficient in reading and mathematics’, i.e. exactly how language proficiency is linked to academic achievement. particularly in rural and township schools that adopt a transitional or ‘english from grade 1’ model, the generative mechanisms are considered to be as follows: a lack of sufficient academic language development in the l1, making the leap from learning the language in the first 3 grades to using it for learning in grade 4 too steep (heugh, 2009); teachers’ inevitable use of code switching, which arguably builds neither the l1 nor l2 (holmarscottir, 2003); and the focus on lower-order cognitive tasks as a way of compensating for lack of mastery of the medium of instruction (fleisch, 2008). another proposed mechanism involves the emotions of l2 teaching and learning. probyn (2001) showed that teachers in township schools found teaching in english to be stressful and felt that the learners were equally affected by the demands of learning in english, in that they often understood what they were learning but could not express themselves, leading to embarrassment and a loss of self-esteem. however, although these factors are important, they are not necessarily the real ‘generative mechanisms’, as fleisch (2008, p. 98) suggests. an analysis of the actual language teaching and learning mechanisms on a psycholinguistic level is more likely to reveal what it is that teachers are teaching and learners are learning about language that may or may not support their academic development. a recent study by meirim, jordaan, kallenbach and rijhumal (2010), for example, examined the development of semantic processing skills such as fast mapping and lexical organisation in a longitudinal study of grade 1 3 eal learners, and suggest that these skills need to be developed at this stage of the education process in order to enhance the learners’ vocabulary acquisition. alternative interpretations of the research on language effects in academic achievement fleisch (2008) points out that although the evidence for language as a major factor contributing to the poor performance of south african children is convincing, the interpretation of these findings needs to be carefully evaluated. he bases this argument on a study by braam (2004), howie’s (2005b) detailed analysis of the timms studies, and a re-appraisal of heugh’s (2000; 2006; 2009) arguments, which are based on earlier research by malherbe (1977). braam (2004) found that in a dual-medium english-afrikaans school on the cape flats, serving a mixed community of lower-middle-class and working-class coloured families, 55% of the children registered in the english stream despite reporting that afrikaans was their home language. across the curriculum, these children were more successful academically than the afrikaans-speaking children enrolled in the afrikaans stream. braam (2004) explains this as reflecting a complex set of class dynamics. in this specific community, afrikaans is stigmatised as the language of the lower class and the teaching practices are aligned to this stigmatisation, with the afrikaans classes subjected to more direct, transmission teaching while the teachers in the english stream, who are also efl speakers, associate english with academic achievement and encourage higher-order thinking. the implication is therefore that home language instruction ‘does not exist in a social and political vacuum’, and teaching in the home language does not necessarily lead to better outcomes (fleisch, 2008, p. 112). howie’s (2005b) analysis of the timms results showed that there are other countries where a large proportion of children (more than 70%) did not speak the language of the test at home (e.g. indonesia, malaysia, morocco, philippines and singapore). in both indonesia and malaysia, a significant proportion of these children did better than children who ‘always’ or ‘sometimes’ spoke the language of the test at home. in singapore, ‘seldom’ or ‘never’ speaking the language of the test at home did not preclude academic excellence. these findings apply not only to east asia but also to countries in north africa, and while the difference on the average mathematics score between children who ‘always’ and ‘never’ spoke the language of the test at home was 157 points in south africa, it was only 46 points in botswana. taken together, these results suggest that language factors are not the only contributors to educational achievement. fleisch (2008) argues that heugh and co-workers, who are advocates of home language instruction in the context of an additive bilingual approach, frequently cite the work of malherbe (1977) on afrikaans-english bilingual schools, showing that children who had afrikaans home language instruction up to grade 7, followed by dual-medium instruction in afrikaans-english from grades 8 12, performed better in both languages than children in monolingual afrikaans or english schools. they also showed higher levels of tolerance for linguistic diversity, and even learning-disabled children performed better. in addition, the dual-medium schools were mostly in rural and less well-resourced areas. heugh (2000) interprets this research as indicating that the african languages should be used as media of instruction for as long as possible, while english is taught as a subject. however, according to fleisch (2008), these findings offer evidence against the ‘home language is best’ position. he cites malherbe’s conclusion that although learning in the l2 results in an initial disadvantage in content subjects, the medium of instruction is less significant as the child progresses to higher grades and eventually has no impact on achievement. malherbe (1977) actually found that the language performance of afrikaans-speaking children in english-medium schools was better than for afrikaans-speaking children in afrikaans schools in the higher grades. it is ironic that fleisch (1995) himself criticised malherbe’s groundbreaking work for its link to a particular political agenda, when in fact it showed the importance and advantage of maintaining both languages, either through using them as the medium of instruction or teaching them as subjects, since the majority of schools in malherbe’s (1977) research were either parallelor dual-medium. this meant that even if the children were being educated in the l2 (english), they still received input in the l1 (afrikaans) at an academic level through subject teaching, and there was continued use of afrikaans in the home environment. of course, comparisons between afrikaans and the african languages as media of instruction should be treated with caution, since the apartheid government invested significant amounts of money in the development of afrikaans, and textbooks, dictionaries, fiction, etc. were readily available. afrikaans also enjoyed high status and was used extensively in the public domain. the support for the home language under these learning conditions makes a difference. the results of a study by morrow, jordaan and fridjhon (2005) contribute some insight into the perceived advantages of bilingual and home language instruction. in this study 181 grade 7 learners from three different contexts (rural, urban and township), where the language-of-instruction practices varied, were tested on an assessment tool constructed in english and translated into isizulu. the assessment tasks were based on the frequency of occurrence of key concepts in a published curriculum package. the learners showed specific patterns of performance dependent on context. the learners in urban ex-model-c schools (taught only in english) performed significantly better in english (89.5%) than in isizulu (58.1%), demonstrating the highest level of competence in english but the lowest in isizulu. learners in townships schools (taught in both english and isizulu) showed similar proficiency in both languages, demonstrating the same level of competence in isizulu (71.43%) and english (73.5%) but significantly higher english scores than the learners in rural schools (taught in both english and isizulu), who did much better in isizulu (75.1%) than in english (53.4%). the study showed that children in the urban schools who were instructed only in english did very well and better than the children in township and rural schools in either english or isizulu, thus contradicting the claim that bilingual education is preferable to monolingual education, and that performance in the l2 is dependent on l1 proficiency, especially since the urban learners obtained a relatively low average score on the isizulu test (58.1%). the children in the township schools demonstrated a balanced profile, but did not do as well in english as their urban counterparts, although they may have caught up at a later stage and would have the advantage of being proficient in both english and isizulu on an academic level. despite receiving instruction in english from grade 4 onwards, the children in the rural schools showed that they were not coping with this medium of instruction and would be far better off if isizulu were used as the language of teaching and learning. the findings of this study thus confirm the strong contextual influences on language in education, and reinforce the conclusion that the role of language in poor school performance is not clear-cut. fleisch (2008) therefore poses the question: do children fail because they do not understand the language of learning and assessment, because of poverty-related issues or because they attend inadequate schools? in all likelihood the answer is an interaction and combination of all these factors, but it is nevertheless important to address the issue of quality, since it is highly variable in different contexts and is considered to be ‘the fundamental problem in south african education …’ (fleisch, 2008, p.121). quality of education and language teaching practices in rural and township schools, the quality of education is affected by five main factors: many teachers are not literate and have poor subject knowledge; the children receive less instructional time because of poor punctuality, absenteeism and preoccupation with other tasks; teachers have low expectations of children; there is poor utilisation of existing materials; and inadequate methods of instruction are used (fleisch, 2008). once again, the extent to which these factors affect urban schools has not been widely researched, but there is some evidence to suggest that there are problems in this system as well. for example, van der sandt and niewoudt (2003) found that grade 7 and prospective student teachers in ex-model-c schools had weak knowledge of geometry. also, in contrast to other countries where teacher expectations are low for certain children, south african teachers are said to have low expectations for all children because of misinterpretation of the grade level requirements of the official curriculum standards and misunderstanding of child-centred pedagogy (vinjefold, 2004, in fleisch, 2008). this results in lower teaching standards and in children becoming complacent about what they know. in particular, and of relevance to this paper, is south african teachers’ knowledge of language and knowledge about language, collectively referred to as ‘teacher language awareness’ (andrews, 2003, p. 81), which directly affects their teaching practices. andrews (2003, p. 84) defines language-teaching practices as the ‘creation of opportunities for language learning in the classroom.’ language-teaching goals and methods may be planned in advance, but the teacher also needs to be flexible and adapt to the discourse demands created by the classroom interaction (wright & bolitho, 1997). according to andrews (2003, p. 86) teachers’ language awareness is ‘metacognitive’, involving the ability to reflect on knowledge of and about language, and this distinguishes the teacher from the learner. this metacognitive dimension of language teaching is central to educational linguistics (brumfit, 1997; reagan, 2009). however, in the international literature as well as in south african research there is evidence to suggest that language in education is a ‘tricky business’ (reagan, 2009, p. vii). educational linguistics is a specialised area that has unfortunately been neglected in teacher-training programmes, and consequently few teachers have sufficient knowledge of the complex, multidimensional nature of language and the implications for language-learning and language-teaching processes in either l1 or l2 contexts (uys, van der walt, van den berg & botha, 2007; o’connor & geiger, 2009; mroz, 2006; wong-fillmore & snow, 2000; andrews, 2003). one of the central issues in educational linguistics is the notion of academic language (wong-fillmore & snow, 2000; cummins, 2008). academic language a recurring theme in the literature on school-age language is the distinction between social and academic uses of language (bailey, 2006; cazden, 2001; chamot, 2005). saville-troike (1984, p. 216) introduced the term ‘academic competence’ to refer to the ‘qualitative difference between the communicative tactics and skills that children find effective for meeting their social needs and goals and those that are necessary for academic achievement in the classroom’. a number of theorists have proposed that the language used in the academic context is qualitatively different from that used in everyday conversational contexts (e.g. bruner’s (1975) communicative and analytic competence, donaldson’s (1978) embedded and disembedded language, olson’s (1977) utterance and text, gibbons’ (1991) playground and classroom language and gee’s (1990) primary and secondary discourses), but a precise description of academic language is elusive (wong-fillmore & snow, 2000) and is dependent on the particular focus of different professional or research communities (valdes, 2004). although the concept of a distinct academic language register can be applied to any language used for teaching and learning, which is an important consideration in the south african context, most of the work in this area has focused on english. for those working with individuals whose l1 is english, academic language refers to literature, writing, language arts, and proficiency in oral and written text, also known as ‘academic discourse’ (valdes, 2004, p. 108). for those working with individuals for whom english is an l2 or additional language, the definition of academic language varies depending on the perspective of the community of practice. the ‘teaching english as a second language’ (tesol) profession views academic language as the language used to carry out academic work at university level as well as the language used by particular disciplines for communication in the field. within this profession, research has focused on english for specific purposes (esp) and english for academic purposes (eap) (bhatia, 1997; johns, 1997; swales, 1990). in contrast, the esl profession working with school-age children defines academic language as language needed to succeed academically in all content areas, including the english used to interact in the classroom and the english used to obtain, process, construct and provide subject matter information in spoken and written form using appropriate learning strategies (valdes, 2004). two approaches are adopted in this community: the teaching of english as a preliminary to instruction in subject matter and clil (coyle, 2008). the bilingual education profession is concerned with the development of academic language in both english and the l1 of students, focusing almost exclusively on cognitive academic language proficiency (calp) in contrast to basic interpersonal communication skills (bics) (cummins, 1984). this distinction was introduced by cummins to explain research findings on bilingual children who appeared to be fluent conversationalists but were still below grade expectations on verbal academic performance in both languages (cummins, 2008). the bics/calp distinction formalised the difference between conversational fluency and academic language as two of, but not the only, conceptual components of the language proficiency construct (cummins, 2008). cummins and yee-fun (2007) distinguish three dimensions of language proficiency: conversational fluency, discrete skills and academic language proficiency. each follows a different developmental trajectory among l1 and l2 children and each responds differently to particular types of instructional practices. conversational fluency is acquired within 1 2 years in face-to-face conversations and uses high-frequency vocabulary and simple grammatical constructions. discrete language skills (listening, speaking, reading and writing) involve learning the rule-governed aspects of language (phonology, grammar and spelling), and are developed by direct instruction and/or immersion in a language-rich home or school environment. these skills can develop concurrently with conversational fluency (weber & longhi-chirlin, 2001) within 2 years (geva, 2000; lesaux & siegel, 2003), but there is little transference to academic language proficiency (kwan & willows, 1998; verhoeven, 2000), which requires more focused teaching. cummins uses calp and academic language proficiency interchangeably to refer to ‘the extent to which an individual has access to and command of the oral and written academic registers of schooling’ (cummins, 2000, p. 67). according to cummins (2008), the distinction should caution educators against conflating the conversational and academic dimensions of proficiency, which may create academic difficulties for children because of the difference in the timelines for the acquisition of conversational and academic language, which depends on language-teaching practices and can take between 5 and 7 years to reach levels commensurate with grade norms. the implications are that students need support in the acquisition of academic language, and in fact scarcella (2009; 2011) claims that with adequate teaching and support, academic language can be acquired more rapidly. the bics/calp distinction was elaborated (cummins, 1984) to show how instructional practices could assist learners to catch up academically. essentially, bics and calp could vary along two dimensions: cognitive demand and contextual support, with the best instructional methods involving context-embedded, cognitively demanding tasks (cummins, 2008). this has implications for teaching quality, which in south africa is often influenced by low teacher expectations and consequently low cognitive demand in the classroom (reddy et al., 2006). cummins (2008), guthrie (2004) and wong-fillmore and snow (2000) suggest that written texts are a reliable source of academic english but need to be presented with instructional support to aid in language development. hence teachers need to help children acquire the academic language register by discussing not only the content but also the language used in texts. teachers can transform text into usable input by helping children to make sense of what they read and drawing attention to how language is used in the materials they are reading (wong-fillmore, 1997). cummins’ model has been criticised (scarcella, 2003; valdes, 2004; edelsky 1990; martin-jones & romaine, 1986; macswan, 2000) as an oversimplification of what constitutes contextual support and cognitive demand and for reflecting a ‘deficit’ perspective (aukerman, 2007) that attributes academic difficulties to low calp. however, the bics/calp distinction can be related to other theoretical distinctions (see bruner, 1975; donaldson, 1978; olson, 1977; gibbons, 1991; gee 1990) and although the terms vary, the basic distinction relates to the extent to which meaning is supported by contextual cues or is primarily linguistic in nature. westby (1994) shows how both the rhetorical (who is talked to) and referential (what is talked about) dimensions of communication become less context-embedded in school. along the rhetorical dimension, children are expected to learn to talk not only to individuals as they did in the preschool period, but also to groups of people, and to both familiar and unfamiliar listeners. this requires more specificity in vocabulary and syntax, since the child cannot depend on shared knowledge and must learn to take the listener’s perspective. along the referential dimension, children no longer talk only to meet their social needs, but must learn to talk about past and future experiences, and to generalise and theorise about these experiences, which involves increasing distance from contextual cues. according to westby (1994), narrative language is particularly important for the development of this decontextualised communication and should be used extensively in the early school years. furthermore wong-fillmore and snow (2000) and cummins and yee-fun (2007) maintain that academic language is challenging for both l2 and l1 learners, since few children start school with the ability to interpret text and do not necessarily have the discourse skills required in education. language-in-education practices in south african classrooms in south africa, the lack of attention to educational linguistics is exacerbated by ill-informed and misunderstood concepts and teaching practices such as ‘whole language’, ‘communicative language teaching’ (heugh, 2009, p. 168) and ‘natural language’, which have become almost ‘orthodoxies’ in the education system (balfour, 2007, p. 6). the communicative approach assumes that language learning only occurs in real-life contexts, where the communicative functions of language drive the acquisition process. teachers do not act as instructors but as facilitators of the process through natural communication and interaction, using comprehensible input of a sufficiently high quality and complexity to ensure that learners will acquire the semantic and syntactic systems of the language of instruction in a subconscious, implicit way (balfour, 2007). this approach is based on krashen’s (1988) distinction between acquisition and learning, which are in fact not ‘distinct and separate’ processes (baker, 2001, in reagan, 2009, p. 59), and are both adequately aligned with a constructivist approach to language learning (reagan, 2009). this approach is discussed in more detail below. furthermore, one of the most disabling effects of obe has been that teachers were encouraged not to teach language and literacy skills explicitly (heugh, 2009), which meant that learners did not receive the necessary scaffolding to develop these skills. teacher-training programmes have similarly de-emphasised explicit teaching because they have had to work within the obe framework because of the substantial financial investment in its introduction and implementation (heugh, 2009). although there have been various attempts to remedy the situation from time to time, e.g. the foundations for learning campaign (tyobeka, 2008), and the minister of basic education’s recent announcement of changes to the obe system (motshekga, 2010), teachers’ language awareness and consequently the power of language in education remains limited. the practice of contentand language-integrated instruction is virtually non-existent, because subject teachers regard language teaching as the responsibility of the language teachers and do not know that they can also teach the language of the subject (uys et al., 2007). the recent introduction of a training module at north-west university, in which student teachers were shown how to implement this approach, is an encouraging development and proves that it can be done within an outcomes-based framework (uys, van der walt, botha & van den berg, 2006). the above discussion leads to the conclusion that perhaps the fundamental problem in education is that language learning should be approached within a ‘constructivist epistemology’ (reagan, 2009, p. 54) that focuses on generative mechanisms and processes, while obe, although claimed to be constructivist (heugh, 2009), has been misunderstood and thus misdirected. it essentially reflects a positivist epistemology (balfour, 2007). social constructivism reagan (2009), in his book language matters, maintains that theories of learning in general and of language learning in particular are examples of metaphors which are culturally determined cognitive tools that shape our thoughts. it is therefore understandable that there would be different philosophies of learning within a multicultural society such as south africa, and that what are essentially western concepts would be misconstrued. to understand what learning is about, and how learning theories have evolved over time, reagan (2009) argues that one should examine some of the philosophies of learning. plato, for example, theorised that learning is basically accessing what one already knows, and according to reagan the socratic teaching method, involving active engagement with learners, is grounded in this idea. in contrast, the philosopher locke posited that the child’s mind is a ‘tabula rasa’ and the ‘teacher pours knowledge into the child’ (reagan, 2009, p. 57). this philosophy was adopted by 19th-century psychologists, who emphasised scientific, observable facts in understanding learning, and the philosophy developed into the behaviourist school of psychology, which was influential in education throughout the 20th century. transmission teaching methods and the audiolingual method in l2 teaching are applications of behaviourist psychology. it is interesting that these teaching methods are still evident in many south african classrooms (fleisch, 2008), despite the introduction of the new curriculum. reagan (2009, p. 59) claims that although cognitive science has changed our understanding of how the human brain learns, there is still a gap between this knowledge and application to classroom practice, and ‘the science of learning has not yet emerged ... we are still reliant on metaphors to understand the nature of learning’. according to this author, constructivism is one of the more powerful metaphors, but has not been investigated extensively in the context of language learning. furthermore, there is no general consensus on the meaning of the concept and whether it is an epistemology, educational philosophy, teaching approach or theory of teaching or learning (reagan, 2009). because metaphors often inform us about what things are not, we do know that constructivism is not a theory of teaching; it is a theory for defining knowledge and learning (fosnat, 1993, in reagan, 2009, p. 62) and it rejects traditional, transmission approaches. constructivism defines knowledge as temporary, developed socially, and mediated culturally. it emphasises the individual learner’s construction of knowledge and the personal nature of learning. one of the principles of learning in constructivism is that the classroom is a discourse community, engaged in reflection, conversation and activity (reagan, 2009). according to reagan (2009), there are two competing types of constructivism: radical constructivism, which is piagetian in orientation and takes a cognitive view of learning, and social constructivism, which is vygotskian and emphasises the sociocultural context of learning as a socially constructed, mediated process. the two types can, however, be reconciled. social constructivism is also entirely compatible with direct instruction, and although it is learner-centred, the content and skills of the learning process are the teacher’s responsibility (reagan, 2009). social constructivism can be applied to language acquisition and learning, if it is correctly interpreted. furthermore, it is compatible with clil (coyle, 2008; gibbons, 2002). broadly defined, clil is instruction in the academic language necessary to accomplish content-area tasks (sherris, 2008), and it has been shown in research to have a positive impact on learning (coyle, 2008). historically, clil stems from the position that l2 proficiency is facilitated by using the language as a medium for learning (mohan, 1986) and it has four key principles of practice: planning content and language goals for each lesson; construction of specific language and content knowledge and skills through interaction; opportunities to develop reading, writing and listening skills within content areas; and assessment of outcomes during lessons (sherris, 2008). social constructivism also informs the practice of speech-language therapists (slts) who work with children who do not acquire their l1 naturally. these professionals have learned that to facilitate language learning it is necessary to create authentic, pragmatically appropriate contexts for communication, as in communicative language teaching, but within this it is essential to set explicit language targets and to use selected elicitation techniques and interaction methods that advance the acquisition of language through scaffolding (owens, 2004). for this reason, slts are well positioned to work in collaboration with teachers to enhance language learning in south african classrooms. two recent studies (wium, louw, & eloff, 2010; olivier, anthonissen & southwood, 2010) confirm that this is not only possible but highly effective. conclusion in the final analysis, and if we acknowledge that one of the primary goals of education is to develop academic language, so that learners may engage meaningfully with the content and subject matter across the curriculum at all stages of the process, it is irrelevant whether the language of learning is the l1 or an additional language, and whether the language is taught as a subject or is the medium of instruction. to achieve academic language proficiency, language-teaching practices that construct the process of learning must be addressed as a matter of urgency. as taylor, vinjevold and muller (2003, p. 65) have stated, the most significant issue for quality in education is: ‘the all pervasive and extremely powerful influence of language which is unambiguously implicated in learning … and the need for pupils to have as good a grasp of the language of teaching and learning as possible.’ well-informed, experienced slts may well be able to assist in achieving this goal, by providing input to teacher-training programmes, collaborating with teachers on setting and developing language-learning goals, and developing academic language through clil within a social constructivist framework. slts must therefore ensure that they are equipped to provide a relevant contribution in the educational context. references alexander, n. (ed.) (2005). mother tongue-based bilingual education in south africa: the dynamics of implementation. cape town: salty print. andrews, s. (2003). teacher language awareness and the professional knowledge base of the l2 teacher. language awareness, 12(2), 81-95. aukerman, m. (2007). a culpable calp: rethinking the conversational/academic language proficiency distinction in early literacy instruction. the reading teacher, 60(7), 626-636. bailey, a.l. (2006). introduction: teaching and assessing students learning english in school. in a.l. bailey (ed.). language demands of school: putting academic english to the test. new haven: yale university press. balfour, r.j. (2007). mother-tongue education or bilingual education for south africa: theories, pedagogies and sustainability. journal for language teaching, 41(2), 1-14. bhatia, v.k. (1997). analysing genre: language use in professional settings. london: longman. braam, d. (2004). community perception of change in a school’s language policy. praesa occasional paper, 21, 33-34. brice, a., & brice, r. (2000). language in the classroom: comparisons of four bilingual environments. the south african journal of communication disorders, 47, 91-98. brock-utne, b., & skattum, i. (2009). languages and education in africa: a comparative and transdisciplinary analysis. oxford: symposium books. broom, y. (2004). reading english in multilingual south african primary schools. bilingual education and bilingualism, 7(6), 506-528. brumfit, c. (1997). the teacher as an educational linguist. in l. van lier & d. corson (eds.). encyclopaedia of language and education, vol. 6: knowledge about language (163-171). new york: springer. bruner, j.s. (1975). language as an instrument of thought. in a. davies (ed.). problems of language and learning (pp. 61-88). london: heinemann. cazden, c. (2001). classroom discourse: the language of teaching and learning (2nd ed.). portsmouth: heinemann. chamot, a.u. (2005). the cognitive academic language learning approach (calla): an update. in p. richard-amato & m.a. snow (eds.). academic success for english language learners. white plains, ny: longman. coyle, d. (2008). clil – a pedagogical approach from the european perspective. in n.v. deusen-scholl & n.h. hornberger (eds.). encyclopedia of language and education: second and foreign language education. new york: springer science. cummins, j. (1984). wanted: a theoretical framework for relating language proficiency to academic achievement among bilingual students. in c. rivera (ed.). language proficiency and academic achievement (p. 10). clevedon: multilingual matters. cummins, j. (2000). language, power and pedadogy. toronto: multilingual matters. cummins, j. (2008). bics and calp: empirical and theoretical status of the distinction. in b. street & n.h. hornberger (eds.). encyclopedia of language and education: literacy (pp. 71-83). new york: springer science. cummins, j., & yee-fun, e.m. (2007). academic language. in j. cummins & c. davison (eds.). international handbook of english language teaching (pp. 797-810). new york: springer. department of education. (2005). grade 6 intermediate phase systematic evaluation report. pretoria: department of education. donaldson, m. (1978). children’s minds. glasgow: collins. du plessis, s., & louw, b. (2008). challenges to preschool teachers in learner’s acquisition of english as language of learning and teaching. south african journal of education, 28, 53-75. du plessis, s., & naude, e.c. (2003). the needs of teachers in preschool centres with regard to multilingual learners. south african journal of education, 23, 122-129. edelsky, c. (1990). with literacy and justice for all: rethinking the social in language and education. london: falmer press. fleisch, b. (1995). social scientists as policy makers: eg malherbe and the national bureau for social and educational research, 1929-1943. journal of southern african studies, 21(3), 349-372. fleisch, b. (2008). primary education in crisis: why south african school children underachieve in reading and mathematics. cape town: juta & co. gee, j.p. (1990). social linguistics and literacies: ideologies in discourses. new york: falmer press. geva, d. (2000). issues in the assessment of reading disabilities in l2 children: beliefs and research evidence dyslexia, 6, 13-28. gibbons, p. (1991). classroom talk and the learning of new registers in a second language. language and education, 12(2), 99-118. gibbons, p. (2002). scaffolding language, scaffolding learning: teaching second language learners in the mainstream classroom. portsmouth, nh: heinemann. guthrie, j.t. (2004). teaching for literacy engagement. journal of literacy research, 36, 1-30. heugh, k. (2000). the case against bilingual and multilingual education in south africa.praesa, occasional papers 6. heugh, k. (2006, march). an evaluation of language education models in africa: findings & recommendations from the unesco institute for education-adea stocktaking report on lingua. paper presented at the international conference on bilingual education, maputo, mozambique. heugh, k. (2009). into the cauldron: an interplay of indigenous and globalised knowledge with strong and weak notions of literacy and language education in ethiopia and south africa. language matters, 40(2), 166-189. holmarscottir, h. (2003, december). literacy in two languages? implementation of language policy in a post-colonial context. paper presented at the neetreed conference, gausdal hoyfjellshotell, norway. howie, s. (2005a). contextual factors in the school and classroom level related to pupils’ performance in mathematics in south africa. educational research and development, 11(2), 123-140. howie, s. (2005b). system-level evaluation: language and other background factors affecting mathematics achievement. prospects, 64(1), 119-157. johns, a.m. (1997). text, role and context: developing academic literacies. cambridge: cambridge university press. jooste, n. (2003). learning through a second language: a comparative study of the performance in reading comprehension and the cognitive-linguistic processes involved in reading comprehension between first-language english learners and second-language english, first-language isixhosa learners at a grade 5 level. unpublished m.a. dissertation, university of cape town. krashen s. (1988). second language acquisition and second language learning. hemel hempstead, herts: prentice hall. kwan, a.b., & willows, d. (1998). impact of early phonics instruction on children learning english as a second language. paper presented at the national reading conference, austin, texas. lesaux, n.k., & siegel, l.s. (2003). the development of reading in children who speak english as a second language. developmental psychology, 6, 1005-1019. macswan, j. (2000). the threshold hypothesis, semilingualism and other contributions to a deficit view of linguistic minorities. hispanic journal of behavioural sciences, 22(1), 3-45. malherbe, e.g. (1977). education in south africa, 19231975. cape town: juta. martin-jones, m., & romaine, s. (1986). semilingualism: a half-baked theory of communicative competence. applied linguistics, 7, 26-38. meier, c. (2005). addressing problems in integrated schools: student teachers’ perceptions regarding viable solutions for learners’ academic problems. south african journal of education, 25(3), 170-177. meirim, g., jordaan, h., kallenbach, m., & rijhumal, m. (2010). development of semantic processes for academic language in foundation phase eal learners. south african journal of communication disorders, 57, 43-50. mohan, b. (1986). language and content. reading, ma: addison-wesley. moloi, m., & strauss, j. (2005). the sacmeq ii project in south africa: a study of the conditions of schooling and the quality of education. retrieved on 24 april 2011 from http://www.sacmeq.org/links.htm morrow, n., jordaan, h., & fridjhon, p. (2005). the effects of educational context on the understanding of linguistic concepts in english and isizulu by grade 7 learners. south african journal of education, 25, 164-169. motshekga, a. (2010). statement by the minister of basic education on the progress of the review of the national curriculum statement. retrieved on 12 august 2010 from www.education.gov.za/linkclick.aspx?fileticket=7hwwplhfufq%3d mroz, m. (2006). teaching in the foundation stage – how current systems support teachers’ knowledge and understanding of children’s speech and language. international journal of early years education, 14(1), 45-61. mtshali, n., & smillie, n. (2011). many of our kids can’t read or write: study shows pathetic state of primary school education. sunday times, june 29. o’ connor, j., & geiger, m. (2009). challenges facing primary school educators of english second (or other) language learners in the western cape. south african journal of education, 29, 253-269. obondo, m.a. (2008). bilingual education in africa: an overview. in j. cummins & n.h. hornberger (eds.). encyclopaedia of language and education (2nd ed., vol. 5, pp. 151-161). new york: springer science. olivier, a.m., anthonissen, c., & southwood, f. (2010). literacy development of english language learners: the outcomes of an intervention programme in grade r. south african journal of communication disorders, 57, 58-65. olson, d.r. (1977). from utterance to text: the bias of language in speech and writing. harvard educational review, 47, 257-281. owens, r.e. (2004). language disorders: a functional approach to assessment and intervention (4th ed.). boston: pearson education. pienaar, m. (2009). multilingualism and educational interpreting: innovation and delivery. language matters, 40(2), 266-267. probyn, m. (2001). teachers’ voices: teachers’ reflection on learning and teaching through the medium of english as an additional language in south africa. international journal of bilingual education and bilingualism, 4(4), 249-266. reagan, t. (2009). language matters. reflections on educational linguistics. usa: information age publishing. reddy, v. (2005). cross-national achievement study: learning from south africa’s participation in the trends in international mathematics and science study (timms). compare, 35(1), 63-77. reddy, v., kanjee, a., diedricks, g., & winnaar, l. (2006). mathematics and science achievement at south african schools in timss 2003. pretoria: hsrc press. saville-troike, m. (1984). what really matters in second language learning for academic achievement? tesol quarterly, 18(2), 199-219. scarcella, r. (2003). academic english: a conceptual framework. in the university of california linguistic minority research institute technical report. santa barbara, ca: university of california. scarcella, r. (2009). academic language for english language learners. retrieved on 5 june 2009 from http://www.colorincolorado.org scarcella, r. (2011). academic language – across school contexts: helping all pupils access the curriculum. paper presented at saaled congress, cape town convention centre, 30 march 2 april. sherris, a. (2008). integrated content and language instruction. centre for applied linguistics digest, september. retrieved on 7 august 2009 from www.cal.org swales, j.m. (1990). genre analysis: english in academic and research settings. cambridge: cambridge university press. taylor, n., vinjevold, p., & muller, j. (2003). getting schools working: research and systemic school reform in south africa. cape town: pearson education south africa. taylor, s., & yu, d. (2008). the importance of socio-economic status in determining educational achievement in south africa. development policy research unit. stellenbosch university, department of economics, working papers. tyobeka, p. (2008). foundations for learning assessment framework foundation phase. south africa: department of education. uys, m., van der walt, j., botha, s., & van den berg, r. (2006). an integrated course for english-medium of instruction teacher trainees in south africa. journal for language teaching, 40(2), 68-86. uys, m., van der walt, j., van den berg, r., & botha, s. (2007). english medium of instruction: a situation analysis. south african journal of education, 27(1), 69-82. valdes, g. (2004). between support and marginalization: the development of academic language in linguistic minority children. international journal of bilingual education and bilingualism, 7, 102-132. van der sandt, s., & niewoudt, h. (2003). grade 7 teachers’ and prospective teachers’ content knowledge of geometry. south african journal of education, 23(3), 199-205. van rooyen, d., & jordaan, h. (2009). an aspect of language for academic purposes in secondary education: complex sentence comprehension by learners in an integrated gauteng school. south african journal of education, 29(2), 271-287. verhoeven, l. (2000). components in early second language reading and spelling. scientific studies of reading, 4, 313-330. webb, v.a., lafon, m., & pare, p. (2010). bantu languages in education in south africa – ongekho akekho! the absentee owner. language learning journal, 37(3), 273-292. weber, r., & longhi-chirlin, t. (2001). beginning in english: the growth of linguistic and literate abilities in spanish-speaking first graders. reading research and instruction, 41, 19-50. westby, c. (1994). communication refinement in school age and adolescence. in o. haynes & b. shulman (eds.). communication development: foundations, processes and clinical applications (pp. 341-383). englewood cliffs, nj: prentice-hall. western cape education department (2004). grade six learner assessment study 2003: final report. cape town: western cape education department. wium, a.m., louw, b., & eloff, i. (2010). speech-language therapists supporting foundation phase teachers with literacy and numeracy in a rural and township context. south african journal of communication disorders, 57, 14-21. wong-fillmore, l. (1997). authentic literature in esl instruction. glenview, il: scott foresman. wong-fillmore, l.w., & snow, c.e. (2000). what teachers need to know about language. usa: clearing house on language and linguistics. wright, t., & bolitho, r. (1997). towards awareness of english as a professional language. language awareness, 6(2&3), 162-170. probing comprehension of h7/questions in an echolalic child john m . panagos, ph.d. (ohio) dept. speech pathology and audiology, kent state university, kent, ohio. summary comprehension of what, where and when wh questions was evaluated in a four-and-one-half year old echolalic boy who displayed in therapy and at home equivocal response to various question forms. deficits of comprehension were traced to an inadequately developed deep structure (particularly adverbials) rather than to limited knowledge of wh transformations. delayed development of semantic rules, as in the case of time distinctions, may block acquisition of those syntactic rules dependent upon them. opsomming begrip van wat, waar en wanneer is by 'n 41/2-jarige eggolaliese seuntjie geevalueer wat tuis en tydens terapie woordelikse nabootsing van vraagvorme gebruik het. 'n gebrek aan begrip ontspring waarskynlik nie soseer uit 'n beperkte kennis van vraagtransformasies nie, as uit 'n dieptestruktuur wat nie toereikend ontwikkel het nie (veral met betrekking tot bywoorde). die aanleer van sintaktiese reels wat van die semantiese ontwikkeling afhanklik is, kan verhinder word deur 'n vertraagde ontwikkeling van semantiese reels (soos in die geval van onderskeiding van werkwoordtye). in the course of evaluating comprehension of wh questions in an echolalic boy named evan we made some interesting clinical observations. when evan first enrolled in our child language program at kent state university at age four years he was hyperactive, inattentive and language delayed. sentences and phrases spoken to him were typically repeated verbatim. a clinician would ask him, "what are these?" and evan would reply with clear pronunciation, "what are these?" spontaneous utterances were fragmented and telegraphic. they suggested a pre-base level of oral syntax development.7 there were also signs of delayed motor development. toilet training had been later than might be expected, preference was shown for the left hand, vocal quality was husky and low-pitched, and gait was at times noticeably irregular. otherwise evan was regarded by all who knew him as a bright and healthy child with normal developmental potential. in addition he had the benefit of warm and caring parents, both with college degrees and considerable understanding of child development. the mother was a teacher of the deaf. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaansc vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, dese/nber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 john μ. panagos after seven months of group and individual language therapy, focusing on expansion of the base grammar and particularly the auxiliary system within it evan had made considerable progress with us. at 4,8 his spontaneous sentences showed a good variety of expanded base strings with several properly applied elementary transformations. here is a sample of evan's sentences drawn from a corpus of spontaneous utterances collected at home and in free play sessions at the clinic5: ( 1 ) 1 already ask jon. (2) we're having those cookies up there. (3) i want to use blue on my caterpillar. (4) albert doesn't talk (elephant puppet). (5) move your chair, i can't. (6) he waiting for me, for evan. (7) there are legs. (8) the head is upside down. (9) want to play with us? (10) is tarah coming? (11) was that john panagos? (12) what did you study? (13) what's on top of that house? (14) what you making? (15) what's that under there? (16) what did you at school, trina? (17) hey you guys, what you doing here? (18) what happen to theresa's nose? (19) where's this go? (20) where's her flu? (21) where's a telephone pole? (22) where's mother goose (a book)? (23) where's shurdon's blue dune buggy? (24) where jeff get those cookies? (25) my mom bought this shirt where? (26) who brought those? (27) who drew on the wall? (28) who draw that? (29) whose draw that truck? . (30) whose papers are these? furthermore, hyperactivity had diminished along with the echolalic behavior. evan could now sit at a table with his clinician for 40 minutes or more, and work through his lessons attentively without simply repeating everything said to him. comprehension of wh questions, however, remained a problem. depending on the type of interrogative involved (what, how, why . ..), responses were either inconsistently correct, or occurred not at all. occasionally a given question (e.g. "what are you doing?") would cause evan to revert to echolalia. although, as mentioned, spoken sentences were now meaningful and syntactically well-formed, inconsistency in answering questions puzzled and journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) wh questions in an echolalic child 25 frustrated evan's parents, and presented a challenge to the clinician on the case. apparently the problem we faced is not an uncommon one in clinical circles. echolic children are found to be particularly sensitive to various question types4 as are younger normal children around the age of three years.2 we thought it was time to probe evan's comprehension of wh questions more carefully. why and //ow.interrogatives were ruled out from the beginning for the reasons that they were structurally and semantically complex, and that evan to the best of our knowledge had never responded to them or used them in spontaneous speech. however, there was sojne evidence from speech samples that he could process what and where questions. although little was known about the when question, it was also of interest to us. the probe was begun with the what question because it seemed to be the most fundamental of the three. with the help of evan's clinician, the evaluation was carried out over a three week period during individual therapy sessions. the probe at the outset certain operating assumptions (ignoring some detail) were made about the what question. first, an intact noun phrase (np) in the base is required for the application of the rule. second, an np can be marked as either human (someone) or nonhuman (something). third, the what morpheme is a kind of synonym for np-nonhuman. fourth, what can replace np-nonhuman in an underlying string. finally, when np substitution occurs in the predicate, the what morpheme is positioned at the beginning of the sentence. assuming the application of auxiliary transposition and do support transformations, the steps from underlying to surface structure can be shown this way: john kicked the ball. ->• john kicked np-nonhuman (something) john kicked what? what did john kick? a similar analysis can be given for the who question which differs from the what form only in that it applies to nphuman rather than to np-nonhuman. furthermore, the transformational principles of the what question are the same for all wh questions in english, hence the much advertised power and generality of transformational rules.2 with the foregoing structural analysis in mind it was hypothesized that failure to comprehend the what question could be caused by: (1) absence of an intact or lexically differentiated underlying np. (2) failure to distinguish nphuman (someone) from np-nonhuman (something), (3) failure to associate what with. np-nonhuman, (4) failure to grasp the what for np substitution, or (5) failure to track the relocation of the what morpheme when moved from the predicate to the beginning of the sentence. to probe these components of comprehension of the what question was the task at hand. it was found that evan could comprehend all aspects of the what question. his clinician placed a group of small objects (car, ball, comb, penny . . ,),on the table in front of him and asked him to name each one in turn. he could do so without difficulty. next the clinician asked, "what?", and simultaneously pointed to one of the objects. accordingly, evan named all of the items flawlessly. then the clinician pointed again to the objects, one at a time, and said, "this is what?", making the what substitution in the predicate. still tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 john μ. panagos evan's naming responses were correct and unhesitating. finally, the inverted form of the what question was asked, "what is this?", and the same pattern of correct response obtained. thus knowledge of the what question within the limits of the tasks used was assumed. evan's parents agreed. they reported his ability to name was exceptional, and that he always answered what questions as well as who questions whenever they were asked of him. inspection of our corpus of spontaneous utterances revealed a number of well-formed what questions involving both subject and predicate contexts (cf. sentences 12-18 above). who questions (sentences 26-28) were also present. next a similar analysis of the where question was performed. the where question relies on the same transformational principles used in the what question, except that the phrase structure context of the replacement process is the place adverbial (somewhere) instead of the noun phrase. from deep to surface structure, then, the progression takes this form: the ball is there (somewhere) the ball is where? where is the ball? a variant form is one in which the place adverbial is a prepositional phrase: the ball is in the box (somewhere) the box is where? where is the ball? in this case the where morpheme replaces an entire phrase with its own internal structure (prep + np). furthermore, the relations between the prepositions and noun phrases designate spatial relations within the conceptual system. the objects used to evaluate comprehension of the what question were used to evaluate the where question. they were divided into two groups and separated spatially. half were placed on the therapy table at which evan was seated, and the other half were positioned on a second table across the room. in a sense evan was asked to name the location of the objects with the adverbials here and there. upon hearing the word "ball", for example, he was required to say "there" because it was located on the distant table. evan could perform this locative task easily, making no errors in the process. the same held true for the sentence forms, "the ball is where?" and "where is the ball?" he responded quickly and accurately to all questions asked. the results here were consonant with those found for the what question. in both cases evan showed the ability to process separately and collectively all phases of wh question derivation. however, when the testing'paradigm required a response with a locative prepositional phrase, the picture changed somewhat. ability to respond correctly depended directly on knowledge of prepositions. with all of the objects placed on the table in front of evan, along with a cardboard box, the clinician selected an object and held or placed it in a position relative to the box. asked "where is the ball?", evan could reply appropriately with "on the box'^and "in the box", but not with "behind the box" or "near the box". many prepositions like near, behind, and between, as well as the phrases in front of, and close-to, were simply unknown to evan, and this was the reason why some where questions were incomprehensible to him. ; examination of the corpus of spontaneous utterances showed a good number of well-formed where questions (cf. sentences 19425 above) to support the findings of our comprehension testing. there was!, interestingly enough, one sentence in which when replacement occurred in the predicate without prejournal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) wh questions in an echolalic child 27 posing (my mom bought this shirt where?). the base included a sufficient number of place adverbials to define the category, but lexical differentiation was limited. prepositional phrases included only six locative prep + np sequences (at-, from-, to-, on-, in-, under-). the when interrogative next evaluated is structurally similar to the what and where questions, except that it elicits from the base information concerning time adverbials. time adverbials can be single words (now, later, today) or phrases (in the evening, last night, in june). transformationally we have, late the cake yesterday (sometime) late the cake when? when did i eat the cake? adverbials of time of course do not make reference to tangible objects, but to abstractions about the time continuum (weber and weber, 1973). this fact makes when questions difficult to evaluate, since, unlike what and where forms, objects cannot be displayed as the context of testing. our only alternative was to query evan about obvious time distinctions (yesterday-today, last-night, this-morning. ..) perhaps known to him from everyday experience. our efforts to probe the when question were short lived. questions like, "when did you watch hockey, evan?", or, "you ate dinner when?" went unheeded. evan would just mumble to himself and look down at the therapy table in front of him. his mother confirmed our observations: "time concepts are really a problem for him. he doesn't seem to remember when he's done something" (like going to his grandfather's). review of the corpus items quickly revealed the problem. it was that evan.did not have a sufficiently developed system of time adverbials with which to understand when questions. there were only two occurrences of time adverbials, both single words and quite likely memorized items (no, i already did (write to santa). he still lives in mansfield?). nor could there be found a single when question. in fact, there was in general little evidence of grammatical productivity of time features. a few strong verbs of past-tense form were located (who drew on the wall? who was that on the phone? my mom bought this shirt where?), as well as one instance of an inflected verb (i burped). in several instances do carried past-tense meaning appropriately (what did you study? what did you at school? . . .). however, many of these utterances had an immediacy to them (i burped now) or reflected only the grossest differentiation of time categorization. temporal adverbials serving as sentence conjunctions (before s, when s) were totally absent from the corpus. all available evidence indicated that evan possessed a highly restricted system of temporal concepts whose contributions to syntactic development were minimal. discussion the findings of our inquiry into evan's comprehension of wh questions can be summarized as follows. adult native speakers around him (parents, clinicians) indicated difficulty in understanding wh questions, but for the most part this judgment proved erroneous. evan had receptive and expressive control over the operations of the wvj/constituent association, wh for constituent replacement, and wh preposing, both as separate steps in the derivation of interrogatives, and combined processes.2 rather what determined and predicted his comprehension performance was the availability or nonavailability of underlying phrasal categories to which particular wh questions referred. tydskrif van die sid-arikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 john μ. panagos noun phrases were well developed lexically and therefore what questions presented no difficulty. adverbials of place were reasonably established in the base, so where questions were not problematic, unless specific prepositions mapping spatial relations were involved. finally, when questions could not be answered, for they were referenced to nonexistent time adverbials or temporal concepts. in short, evan's comprehension difficulty lay less with comprehending questions than it did in understanding particular aspects of the deep structure of the language. in 1965, chomsky3 revised his theory of generative grammar to include a semantic component having the purpose of interpreting the meaning of syntactic strings. then and since then, there has been considerable debate about the nature and scope of the semantic component and its influence on syntactic rules. that the two components have reciprocal influences on one another in early language acquisition was insightfully demonstrated by bloom1 and further clarified by macnamara.6 some recent evidence dealing with the acquisition of time designations demonstrates the independence and uniqueness of semantic systems.8 these advances in semantic theory are applicable here. our hyperactive and echolalic friend evan, with his perceptual inclination to be bound to the here-and-now, seemed to lack adequate semantic acquisition to support aspects of his syntax development. most striking was his inadequate knowledge of temporal concepts relating to time adverbials in the base of his syntax grammar. it could be hypothesized that the absence of a semantic system of time concepts was blocking growth at several levels of syntactic development. with figure 1 the ways in which this influence might w h e n evan past eat dinner yesterday semantic distinction time = present/past syntactic coding: a,b,c,d.. adv-time past go figure 1. hypothetical phrase marker representation showing various syntactic contexts (a-d) in which the present/past semantic distinction is coded. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) wh questions in an echolalic child 29 be manifested are illustrated. it can be seen that the nonacquisition of the semantic distinction of present/past could simultaneously affect development of the when question (a), tense marking of the main verb (b), time adverbials within the basefc), and embedded strings derived from time adverbials (d). we have therefore a semantic deficit affecting the distinctly different syntactic processes of morpheme inflection, phrase structure categorization, simple transformations, and generalized transformations. certainly semantic rules about which little is known can have subtle and far-reaching effects on the syntax development of normal children, and on language-impaired children like evan. acknowledgments my thanks to roberta bronahan, sheila higgins, judy badovinac, sue williams, and patricia dukes for sharing sentence data with me, and to candice wice who was the clinician on the case and helped with the evaluation reported here. special thanks go to my friends tom and jane, evan's parents, and to evan himself, who continues to be a delightful fellow to know. references 1. bloom, l. (1970): language development. cambridge, mass.: m.i.t. press. 2. brown, r. (1968): the development of wh questions in child speech. j. verb. learn. verb. behav., 7, 279-290. 3. chomsky, n. (1965): aspects of the theory of syntax. cambridge, mass.: m.i.t. press. 4. fay, w.h. (1973): occurrence of children's echoic responses according to interlocutory question types. unpublished manuscript, university of oregon medical school. 5.. higgins, s.m. (1973): normal and disordered child language: grammatical analysis and comparison. unpublished seminar paper, kent state university. 6. macnamara, j. (1972): cognitive basis of language learning in infants. psychol. rev., 79, 1-13. 7. menyuk, p. (1969): sentences children use. cambridge, mass.: m.i.t. press. 8. weber, j., and weber, s. (1973): early acquisition of linguistic designations for time. unpublished paper delivered at american speech and hearing association convention, detroit, michigan. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acoustics and signal analysis noise problems? general radio has the solution: a complete family of noise measuring gri565-b i i instruments for community and industrial noise: for more information please contact: associated electronics (pty.) ud. ro.box 3 1 0 9 4 , phone 7 2 4 5 3 9 5 braamfontein, johannesburg general radio journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. , december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 vocal t r a c t dynamics in an a d u l t stutterer lesley wolk, b . a . ( s p & η therapy) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) , department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary the present study was motivated by the clinical observation of "laryngeal spasms" during dysfluency in an adult female stutterer. the flexible fiberoptic nasolaryngoscope was employed in an attempt to assess this phenomenon objectively. findings from fiberscopic and spectrograph^ investigations provided evidence for a disturbance in laryngeal behaviour, and in turn served to determine the nature of the treatment programme. asymmetry of the vocal folds and partial abductory laryngeal behaviour, reflecting a conflict between adductory and abductory forces, characterized the dysfluency in this patient. a subjective evaluation after treatment revealed a reduction in both severity and frequency of stuttering behaviour. furthermore, fiberscopic examination carried out after treatment revealed an absence of the laryngeal disturbances noted previously. results are considered in terms of vocal tract dynamics in stuttering and its clinical applicability. opsomming die kliniese waarneming van "laringeale spasmas" gedurende onvlotheid in 'n volwasse vroulike hakkelaar het die navorser beweeg tot verdere ondersoek van hierdie verskynsel. 'n buigbare fiberskopiese nasolaringoskoop is gebruik in 'n poging om die verskynsel objektief te evalueer. bevindings van fiberskopiese en spektografiese ondersoeke het bewys gelewer van 'n versteuring in laringeale gedrag, en het terselfdertyd ook die aard van die terapieprogram bepaal. asimmetrie en gedeeltelike abduksie van die stemlippe wat 'n konflik tussen adduktor en abduktor kragte reflekteer, was 'n kenmerk van die pasient se onvlotheid. 'n subjektiewe evaluasie na behandeling het 'n vermindering in beide die graad en frekwensie van hakkelgedrag getoon. 'n fiberskopiese ondersoek, wat na die behandeling uitgevoer is, het verder 'n afwesigheid van laringeale versteuring getoon. resultate is gei'nterpreteer teen die agtergrond van vokale kanaal dinamiek in hakkel en die kliniese toepassing daarvan. many years ago it was theorized that the larynx plays a significant role in stuttering (kenyon9). recently, investigations have provided some evidence for a relationship between phonatory behaviour and stuttering; the nature of which however, is still unclear (adams and hayden, 1 conture, mccall and brewer, 2 freeman and ushijima3, inter alia). modern technological advances have facilitated the use of several physiological measurement strategies in research with stutterers (hutchinson8). to date, none of these techniques has received more than isolated research interest. furthermore, only little significant effort has been made to extend these experimental results to their practical clinical utility.. this study was motivated by the clinical observation of "laryngeal spasms" ι associated with dysfluency, during the course of therapy with an adult female stutterer. the flexible fiberoptic nasolaryngoscope (fiberscope) was thus employed to investigate the phonatory gestures in this patient. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 39 vocal tract dynamics in stuttering m this paper, the author's intention i s : (a) to discuss the fiberscopic ^ n a t i o n in the ongoing assessment of this adult stutterer; (b) to s describe the treatment programme employed and (c) to discuss the overall findings in terms of vocal tract dynamics in stuttering and to c o n s i d e r some practical implications. background to subject history of speech disorder: mrs. a, aged 40 years, was aft seen at the university speech and hearing clinic, university of the witw a t e r s r a n d , johannesburg, on the 21st march 1979 mrs a reported that she had stuttered since childhood, which her mother associated with the bombing during the war. she was born in eneland and lived there for many years until recently when her husband was transferred to south africa on a contract with a diamond mining company. about 18 months prior to the initial assessment mr. a suffered a traumatic head injury which resulted in bilateral deafness. mrs a stated that her stutter deteriorated noticeably since her husband's accident. at three stages of her life, mrs. a received short periods of speech therapy, although on all these occasions she felt that she had not benefited. medical history: she suffered from common childhood illnesses and underwent surgery for an eye defect. no other significant illnesses were reported. psychological a n d family history: mrs. a reported that she experiences much social discomfort since the onset of her husband s hearing impairment and the deterioration of her speech she is said to share a warm relationship with her husband and three daughters. two of her children are reported to stutter mildly. assessment of communication b e h a v i o u r the following areas were assessed by the author before the onset of treatment. results are summarized in table i. description of stuttering behaviour mrs a was classified as an advanced stutterer. her speech was characterized by frequent prolongations on fricatives, and several hard contacts on plosives, nasals and affricates in all positions hard contacts seemed to occur most frequently on voiceless plosives /p, t, k/ appearing as tense, forceful attacks combined with a severe remor. the tension loci were in the lips, tongue, cheeks and laryngeal areas. this nonfluent speech pattern appeared to be habituated and consistent — some adaptation was noted. there was evidence of severe facial tension, characterized by eye closure, eye rolling, frowning, jaw jerks, tremor of lips and flaring of nostrils upward movement of the head was noted to occur, as well as die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) lesley wolk t a b l e i: summary of findings from an assessment of communication behaviour hearing — within normal limits oral peripheral examination — structural competence — functional adequacy for speech language — normal receptive and expressive language skills voice — soft, high-pitched voice articulation — normal rhythm, stress, phrasing — interrupted laterality — established right (r) dominance motor co-ordination — adequate clinical observations — reserved personality — anxious and tense, pleasant disposition — insightful awareness and attentativeness some movement of the extremities. there was associated disturbance of the respiratory mechanism such as pauses, gasping and frequent inhalations. she was often seen to cease a speech attempt and try again, and to employ starters with a sudden increase in tension and force. after a severe block, there was evidence of humiliation and intense anxiety. eye contact was extremely poor. choral speaking, singing, whispering and diminished auditory sensitivity were found to induce fluent speech. she was able to isolate several word and situation fears; the telephone being the most anxiety-provoking situation for her. diagnostic investigations o f l a r y n g e a l beh a v i o u r fiberscopic assessment the fiberscope was employed to . observe laryngeal behaviour directly during connected speech for the purpose of providing objective data that would assist in evaluating the role of the larynx in the dysfluency of this adult stutterer. ! procedure ; the patient's nasal passages and ι posterior aspects of the pharyngeal wall were topically anaesthetized! to prepare for the insertion of the fiberscope. it was connected by a c-mount adapter to a camera. a the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vocal tract dynamics in stuttering 41 diagnostic fiberoptic light source provided necessary illumination. the fiberscope was inserted into one nostril until the tip of the scope reached a level slightly rostral to the superior margin of the epiglottis. a tape recorder, uher 4200 report stereo i.c. with an m816 directional microphone, was prepared to run concurrently with this examination so as to record all utterances. a transcription of the tape recordings was carried out as each utterance was correlated with its corresponding slide. there was a total of 38 slides. broad-band spectrographic analyses with amplitude displays were carried out on each of these utterances in order to segment the duration of exposure, thus confirming whether the photograph was taken at the moment of the stuttered block. it was always entirely clear, both from broad-band spectrograms and the amplitude displays when the shutter had opened and closed. for the purpose of this paper, only a few of these utterances were selected in order to highlight the major findings. results figures 1(a) and (b) show that the vocal cords are normal at rest and during deep inhalation respectively. figure 2(a) shows a stuttering block on /k/ in the word "cancellations". from observation during fiberscopic examination, it appeared that there was asymmetry of the vocal folds relative to each other. posteriorly at the" vocal processes, the left (l) cord is more adducted than the right (r) cord. there is also clear asymmetry of the arytenoid cartilages. it can be seen that there is a conflict between adductory and abductory forces, which may be referred to as "partial abductory" laryngeal behaviour. the same asymmetry appears as the standard picture of a characteristic block on other plosive sounds (voiced and voiceless) in initial and medial positions. the broad-band spectrogram in figure 2(b) confirms the fact that this photograph was taken at the moment of the stuttered block. the first noise burst, marked s(o) on the spectrogram, refers to when the shutter opened and the second noise burst, marked s(c), refers to when the shutter closed. in figure 3(a) similar partial abductory laryngeal behaviour can be seen during a sound prolongation on /s/ in the word "sweater". the broad-band spectrogram in figure 3(b) provides confirmation as in figure 2(b) above. during observation of the vocal cords, there was evidence of epiglottic movement and shuddering of the entire larynx during dysfluency. this figure clearly illustrates the standard picture of asymmetry where the (r) fold appears more prominently than the (l) one. the distances between the vocal cords were measured. results revealed that the distance posteriorly was 5 units of measurement, medially 3 units and anteriorly 4 units*. the anterior measurement was taken at the point where the folds seem to shade off, i.e. •it is not claimed that these are absolute glottal widths, as the degree of magnification has not been computed, but the proportions are correct. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 lesley wolk anterior m e a s u r e m e n t w h e r e cords lose definition notice that there is medial adduction which is abnormal. thus the narrowest portion of the glottis is medial, while the posterior and anterior portions are both wider, reflecting a disturbance in normal largyngeal behaviour. the striking feature was this recurring picture of arching, which will be referred to as the "arched effect". in summary, a review of the tape recordings and corresponding slides, together with measurements of the glottis calculated for each utterance, revealed the following: 1. the vocal folds were normal at rest and during deep inhalation. they were parallel and symmetrical during all fluent utterances and during singing. 2. there was a general picture of shuddering of the larynx during dysfluency. a figure 1(a) subject mrs. a's laryngeal behaviour during quiet breathing, showing that the vocal cords are normal at rest. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vocal tract dynamics in stuttering 43 3. there was evidence of asymmetry of the arytenoids and vocal cords during all dysfluent utterances, where the (r) fold appeared more prominently than the (l) one. 4. partial abductory laryngeal behaviour referred to as the "arched effect", characterized all dysfluency, stuttering blocks and prolongation in all positions. the identical picture was observed with voiced and voiceless fricatives, affricates and plosives. β figure 1(b) subject mrs. a's laryngeal behaviour during deep inhalation, showing that the vocal cords are normal during deep breathing. a β figure 2(a) laryngeal behaviour during a stuttering block on /k/ in "cancellations". the line-tracing which overlays b, outlines the laryngeal structures shown during this production. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 lesley wolk 5(o> sco i 4 κ < 9'ψ fna* «̂ί» is mte..ti «rati figure broad-band spectrogram showing a block on dysfluent production of /k/ in "cancellations" before treatment. a β figure 3(a) laryngeal behaviour during prolongation on /s/ in "sweater". the line-tracing which overlays β outlines the laryngeal structures shown during this production. 5co) sco . ji i f'j.». riit^e-jii >1 .-sri-1 til , i ι it·:11 itm ι· λ λ ι μ ι ma λμλβλ» η. a -·; • ' • % si ί μ t αΐκ • ·•· figure 3(b) broad-band spectrogram showing a prolongation on dysfluent production of /s/ in "sweater" before treatment. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vocal tract dynamics in stuttering 45 discussion abductory laryngeal behaviour during stuttering is reported in several recent studies (conture et a l 2 ) . however, the specific picture of a s y m m e t r y ("arched effect") reflecting a conflict between adductory and abductory forces, characteristic of the dysfluency in this subject, has not been reported in the literature. in contrast to the present data, conture et al 2 found differences in laryngeal behaviour among the various types of stutterings. they found a separation of the posterior aspects of the vocal folds during part-word repetitions, but normal laryngeal behaviour in sound prolongations. literature on normal vocal cord function in man shows that:— adduction of the vocal cords for phonation is due to the contraction of the vocalis (voc), lateral cricoarytenoid (lca) and interarytenoid (int) muscles; whilst abduction is due to the contraction of the posterior cricoarytenoid (pca) muscle (hiroto, hirano, toyozumi and shin 7 ). the v o c muscle and to a lesser degree the lca muscle regulate the style of phonation. they are active in the medial compression of the vocal cords to produce inner tension of the cords. three "styles of phonation" are generally considered: hypertense, hypotense and optimum styles (freeman et a l 3 ) . with regard to complexity of laryngeal movements preparatory to vocalization, three types of vocal attack are commonly described (a) soft attack (simultaneous) where pca activity is suppressed throughout the pre-phonatory period; lca and v o c activity increase gradually until it reaches a constant level shortly before or after the vocal onset, (b) hard attack (glottal) where pca activity decreases well before the onset of voicing, shows a transient increase in activity just before the onset of voicing and is then suppressed for the period of voicing; there is a high level of lca and v o c activity, presenting forceful action potentials before phonation, and (c) breathy attack (aspirate) where pca remains active throughout the pre-phonatory period up to the point immediately before the onset of voicing. lca and v o c activity is often decreased temporarily before phonation. therefore, the type of vocal attack depends on vocal cord placement at the initiation of voicing, which is determined by the precise co-ordination between sub-glottic pressure, glottal resistance and supraglottic pressure. (gay, strome, hirose and sawashima5). it thus seems reasonable to postulate that in this adult stutterer, there may be higher levels of v o c and/or lca muscle activity (adductors) and inappropriate functioning of the pca muscle (abductor), resulting in a disturbance of the interplay between agonist and antagonist intrinsic laryngeal muscles during stuttering. the fact that the cords are abducted posteriorly would indicate that there is adequate functioning of the interarytenoid muscles. the v o c and lca muscles are thus considered responsible for the relative hypertonicity of the adductors. these findings are in accordance with current research (conture et a l , 2 freeman and ushijima3). it appears that the difficulty may lie in the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 lesley wolk pre-phonatory and/or re-phonatory phase, whereby voicing is initiatied with hard vocal attacks resulting in a hypertense style of phonation. there is evidence in the literature to support the belief that some stutterers fail to produce adequate voluntary pre-setting of the phonatory musculature, and that there is a lack of co-ordination between respiration, articulation and phonation in stuttering (gautheron, liorzou, even and vallancien, perkins, rudas, johnson and bell 1 1 ). w y k e 1 7 and others would explain findings such as these in terms of the reflex arc, whereby higher centres control laryngeal reflex activity. according to this model, psychological stresses affect the supramedullary centres which fail to inhibit pca muscle activity, producing abductive responses to the larynx during stuttering. treatment: a treatment programme incorporating the psychological aspects as well as modification of the faulty laryngeal adjustments developed from the diagnostic findings discussed above. only a brief discussion of this programme will be included in this paper. set within a van r i p e r 1 5 framework, various techniques were employed within the modification phase of therapy. these included van r i p e r ' s 1 5 shaping processes, "prolonged speech" (perkins1 0), "passive-airflow technique" (schwartz1 3), "precision fluency shaping" as discussed by w e b s t e r 1 6 and "modification of vocal attack" developed by the author. modification of vocal attack considering hard attack to be the error pattern with high levels of voc and lca muscle activity and significantly earlier voice onset time, one may view soft attack as the target behaviour representing optimal muscular activity. in view of the fact that breathy attack reduces voc and lca muscle activity temporarily before phonation, thus facilitating gradual voice onset, it was employed as a therapeutic strategy (gautheron et al 4 ). furthermore, there is continuous pca muscle activity during the pre-phonatory period in breathy attack, thus preventing jerky "on-off" vocal adjustments. breathy phonation was combined with a low pitch in order to further reduce electrical activity during phonation (gay et al 5 ). a marked reduction in dysfluency was noted to occur when this method was employed in a variety of speech tasks. / a further technique, biofeedback, was applied. the underlying principle of biofeedback training is that a subject can learn to exert some control over a physiological process if he receives immediate, accurate (auditory or visual) information about the process (hanna, wilfling and mcneill6). briefly, emg activity was recorded from the laryngeal and facial areas where there was excessive tension. a pair of surface electrodes were placed on the skin over the levator labii superioris. the action potentials from this muscle were amplified and the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vocal tract dynamics in stuttering 47 i n t e g r a t e d into a voltage-to-frequency converter. as the electrical activity of the muscles increased, so did the frequency of the emg analog tone. mrs. a was provided with a baseline or reference frequency. she was then presented with a tone whose frequency was analogous to the voltages recorded from the electrodes and her task was to concentrate on lowering this tone, hence lowering the muscular hypertonicity. a second electrode pair was placed over the orbicularis oris and a third electrode pair over the cricothyroid region below the thyroid notch. each time the same procedure was carried out. an informal assessment during the sessions and a subsequent replay of the tape recordings demonstrated that there was a significant reduction in laryngeal e m g activity with a concomitant increase in fluency. no conclusions can be drawn from this preliminary investigation. however, it is conceivable that after sufficient training, a stutterer could learn to attend to somesthetic cues of laryngeal tension in the absence of biofeedback equipment. it is of interest to note that in the study of gautheron et al 4 , their stutterers were encouraged to start phonating breathily while attempting to obtain auto-correction with the aid of visual feedback. in summary, gaining conscious control of speech production by reconstructing articulatory details, enhancing proprioceptive awareness, encouraging smooth transition of laryngeal adjustments and altering the tension of the vocal cord musculature, seems to be the features common to the modification techniques employed in this treatment programme. results of treatment a subjective evaluation after treatment revealed a significant reduction in both severity and frequency of stuttering behaviour. there appeared to be very little facial, neck and laryngeal tension or associated movements. furthermore, there was evidence of improved self-concept and personal adjustment. towards the end of treatment, a further fiberscopic examination was carried out in order to compare mrs. a's laryngeal behaviour before and after treatment. the entire procedure followed that described above. there was a total of 141 utterances (from spontaneous speech) with corresponding slides. figure 4(a)-a shows a stuttering block on /k/ in "cancellations" before treatment, reflecting the asymmetrical "arched effect" with partial abductory laryngeal behaviour. figure 4(a)-b shows a stuttering block on ikj in " k e e n " after treatment. here it can be seen that the glottis is wide open for the voiceless plosive /k/ and there is an absence of the asymmetrical "arched effect" seen in (a). similarly in figure 5(a)-β it can be seen that there is an absence of the arching in the sound prolongation /s/ in "this sea" after treatment. the apparent asymmetry is not due to the original "arched effect", but is due to the asymmetry of the false cords. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) figure 4(a) laryngeal behaviour on (a), a stuttering block on fkj in "cancellations" before treatment, and (b), a stuttering block on /k/ in "keen" after treatment. 5(0) sco figure 4(b) broad-band spectrogram showing a block on dysfluent production of /k/ in " k e e n " after treatment. in each case, broad-band spectrograms confirmed that the.photographs were taken at the instance of the stuttered block (see figures 2(b), 3(b), 4(b) and 5(b)). the fibrescopic representations described here were chosen at random for the purpose of this discussion. however, these representations are characteristic of the standard dysfluencies (stuttering blocks and prolongations) throughout all utterances analysed after treatment. an obvious limitation should be considered in the interpretation of these results. increased tension may have been created by the initial the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vocal tract dynamics in stuttering 49 figure 5(a) laryngeal behaviour on (a), a sound prolongation /s/ in "sweater" before treatment, and (b), a sound prolongation /s/ in "this sea" after treatment. s(o) $co figure 5(b) broad-band spectrogram showing prolongation on dysfluent production of /s/ in "this sea" after treatment. note: the peak value of pca laryngeal muscular activity is the same for /s/ sounds in different positions, initially and medially (hirose and ushijima, 1974). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 lesley wolk fiberscopic procedure resulting in increased stuttering, and conversely the familiarity of the procedure after'treatment may have reduced stuttering. however, qualitative assessments of the tape recordings during both procedures were found to be representative when compared with tape recordings of her speech outside of the fiberscopic investigations. conclusions a n d implications the present findings support the contention that laryngeal disturbances are associated with many moments of stuttering (conture et al2). while this may explain the proximal cause of stuttering, we are still faced with the question of "primary causation". in a recent study, schmitt and c o o p e r 1 2 found no laryngeal abnormalities in a group of young stutterers. this leads one to question whether the differences observed in the phonatory behaviour of adult stutterers might be more a reflection of habituated compensatory phonatory adjustments in response to dysfluencies than they are indicators of an etiological key to stuttering. while reference has been made to some underlying neurologic fault in some stutterers, (for example, faulty laryngeal innervation, flaws in temporal lobe sequencing, a deficit in auditory feedback systems or aberrant inter-hemispheric relationships (wyke, 1 7 travis 1 4 ), there is also convincing information that psychological factors must be considered. thus a multidimensional etiology of the manifestations of stuttering behaviour as suggested by van r i p e r 1 5 still seems plausible. the unresolved etiology of stuttering should not, however, restrict the continued search for treatment methods. the benefits of intensive stuttering therapy in this case are clearly indicated. these findings pertain to a single case of stuttering. they are thus tentative and should be regarded as an indication for further research. future investigations in biofeedback as a treatment for stuttering is strongly indicated. thus, an attempt to integrate research on laryngeal dysfunction in stuttering with learning-theory and servo-theory might well be of clinical value. acknowledgements the author wishes to thank mrs. m. wahlhaus, senior lectiirer of the department of speech pathology and audiology, and ms. c. penn, lecturer of the department of speech pathology and'audiology, of the university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, for their guidance and support; dr. a. traill, senior lecturer of the department of phonetics and general linguistics for his valuable assistance in the spectrographic and fiberscopic testing of this patient; dr. e. gordon and dr. a. gordon for their assistance with the biofeedback procedure. the author also acknowledges the human sciences research council for its financial assistance. ι the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vocal tract dynamics in stuttering 51 references 1. adams, m. r. and hayden, p. (1976): the ability of stutterers and non-stutterers to initiate and terminate phonation during production of an isolated vowel. j. speech, hear. res. 19, 290-296. 2. conture, e. g., mccall, g. n. and brewer, d. w. (1977): laryngeal behaviour during stuttering. j. speech. hear. res. 20, 6 6 1 6 6 8 . 3. freeman, f. j. and ushijima, t. (1978): laryngeal muscle activity during stuttering. j. speech. hear. res. 21(3), 538-562. 4. gautheron, b., liorzou, α., even, c. and vallacien, b. (1973): the role of the larynx in stuttering. in: neurolinguistic approaches to stuttering: proceedings of the international symposium on stuttering. le β run, y. and hoops, r. (eds) mouton, the hague. 5. gay, t., strome, h., hirose, h. and sawashima, m. (1972): electromyography of the intrinsic laryngeal muscles during phonation. ann. otol. rhinol. laryngol. 81, 401-408. 6. hanna, r., wilfling, f. and mcneill, b. (1975): a biofeedback treatment for stuttering. j. speech. hear. dis. 40, 270-273. 7. hiroto, i., hirano, m., toyozumi, y. and shin, t. (1967): electromyographic investigation of the instrinsic laryngeal muscles related to speech sounds. ann. otol. rhinol. laryngol. 76, 861-872. 8. hutchinson, j. m. (1974): aerodyamic patterns of stuttered speech. in: vocal tract dynamics and dysfluency. webster, l. m. and furst, l. (eds). speech and hearing institute, n.y., 71-88. 9. kenyon, e. l. (1943): the etiology of stammering: the psychophysiologic facts which concern the production of speech sounds and stammering. j. speech. dis. 8, 337-348. 10. perkins, w. h. (1979): from psychoanalysis to disco-ordination. in: controversies about stuttering therapy. gregory, η. h. (ed). university park press, baltimore. 11. perkins, w. h., rudas, j., johnson, l. and bell, j. (1976): stuttering: disco-ordination of phonation with articulation and respiration. / . speech. hear. res. 19, 509-521. 12. schmitt, l. s. and cooper, ε. b. (1978): fundamental frequencies in oral reading behaviour of stuttering and non-stuttering male children. j. of comm. dis. 11, 17-23. 13. schwartz, m. f. (1976): stuttering solved. (part ii) the treatment. lippincott. j. b. co., redwood, burn. ltd. trowbridge and escher. 14. travis, l. e. (1978): the cerebral dominance theory of stuttering 1931-1978. j. speech. hear. dis. 43, 278-281. 15. van riper, c. (1973): the treatment of stuttering. prentice-hall inc., englewood cliffs, n.j. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 lesley wolk 16. webster, r. l. (1977): a few observations on the manipulation of speech response characteristics in stutterers. in the problems of stuttering — theory and therapy. reiber, r. w. (ed). elsevier north-holland, inc. n.y., 73-76. 17. wyke, b. (1974): phonatory reflex mechanisms and stammering folio phoniat. 26, 321-338. books books campus bookshop westdene services 34 bertha st, braamfontein 76a king george st, hillbrow j h b 3 9 1 7 1 1 j h b 3 9 1 7 1 1 / university medical the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 28, 1981 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 25 teacher accuracy in the identification of pre-school pupils with hearing loss nicole chambers and ilona anderson department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract teachers of 110 pre-school pupils used a questionnaire to identify which children they thought would fail a hearing screening. following screening, the data was compared to both audiometric only, and combined audiometric and tymponometric screening results. teachers identified one out of six pupils who failed audiometric screening, and one out of seven who failed combined screening. we concluded that teachers could not accurately identify pupils with hearing problems and should not be used to detect hearing losses in pupils without prior education and training. opsomming onderwyseresse van 110 voorskoolse hinders is gevra om 'n vraelys te voltooi en aan te dui watter kinders hulle sou identifiseer as die wat nie 'n gehoorsifting sou slaag nie. hierdie resultate is vergelyk met die siftingsresultate ten opsigte van slegs oudiometrie sowel as 'n kombinasie van oudiometrie en timpanometrie. een uit ses hinders, wat nie die oudiometriese siftinggeslaag het nie, en een uit sewe kinders wat nie die gekombineerde siftinggeslaag het nie is deur die onderwyseresse geidentifiseer. die gevolgtrekking was dat onderwyseresse nie kinders met gehoorprobleme kon identifiseer nie en dus nie sonder verdere inligting en opleiding as betroubare identifiseerders gebruik kon word nie. keywords: teachers, pre-school children, hearing loss, identification introduction undetected hearing loss in children can be of detrimental consequence. hearing-impaired children may not receive adequate auditory, linguistic or social stimulation necessary for speech and language learning, social development and emotional development (nih consensus statement, 1993). even a mild loss can place a child at risk for language and learning problems; place an unbearable strain on their coping abilities; and put them at a disadvantage in the classroom (roeser & downs, 1988; davis, elfenbein, schum & bentler, 1986; menchor & mcculloch, 1970). furthermore, entire family functioning may be affected by a child's hearing loss (nih consensus statement, 1993). hearing loss is a problem of significant magnitude and ranks as the commonest form of sensory deprivation (swart, 1995; 1996a). considering the magnitude of the problem of hearing-impairment and its detrimental consequences it becomes clear that early intervention is of critical importance. in developed countries universal screening is usually implemented (swart, 1996b). unfortunately the adoption of such an approach in south africa is unlikely due to problems of inadequate resources, lack of services, lack of facilities, inadequate technology for the underprivileged majority, and lack of personnel (swart, 1995). hearing impairment and deafness affect at least 3 million individuals in south africa (swart, 1995); yet there are only 1094 registered speech and hearing therapists and 5 registered audiologists to provide services for these individuals (samdc, 1997). there are also a number of primary health care workers, nurses and speech and hearing diplomats who are trained to administer hearing screening testing (roeser & downs, 1988) but they are also too few in number to service the entire population. primary health care professionals have little training in the early identification and management of hearing-impairment and ear disease (swart, 1995), and nurses already have a high work-load with which they need to cope. thus the implementation of hearing screening programmes in south africa is problematic. in spite of these problems we need to extend hearing screening services to the whole community. a screening method needs to be developed that is easy to teach, learn and administer. it should be cost-effective, be developed with consideration of the context in south africa, be linguistically and culturally appropriate (child health policy group, 1996), and not require expensive training or high level salaries (northern & downs, 1991). one such approach which is user friendly and fits these criteria is the questionnaire approach (swart, 1996a). questionnaires incorporating at-risk registers have been used quite extensively with neonates, however, they have been used less frequently for the screening of the pre-school population. there have been reports of a slight incidence peak of otitis media within the two to seven year age range, possibly due to increased infection at entry to pre-school or school. furthermore, the two to five year age range gives us problems in identification because the children are ofdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 nicole chambers & ilona anderson ten not seen by doctors or clinics (northern & downs, 1991). the use of a questionnaire as a screening method for this age group could therefore, be especially valuable. at-risk registers would incorporate risk factors that are different from those used for neonates. unlike neonates, pre-school children have begun to use speech and language. delay, deviance or regression in their use of speech and language could serve as new at-risk behaviours for preschoolers. many authors emphasize that the first signs of hearing loss in children are changes in behaviour, learning and language (savary & ferron, 1982). the recognition of these changes as at-risk behaviours could serve as an important screening test. those individuals who have frequent contact with children should be utilized as key personnel for the screening of disabilities (child health policy group, 1996). this would include parents or other caregivers, pre-school teachers, community health workers, traditional healers, nurses and doctors. unfortunately, there is a significant gap of time between the age of eighteen months and five years, where children are often not seen by health professionals (child health policy group, 1996), thus utilizing community health workers, traditional healers, nurses or doctors in the screening process may not be very efficient for children in this age range. parents or caregivers, and pre-school teachers usually do have frequent contact with children in this age group and as a result, could prove useful in administering questionnaires and at-risk registers as part of the hearing screening process during this period. teachers were found to be able to identify one out of four (curry, 1950) or one out of six (kodman, 1956) children who had a hearing loss. nodar (1978) found similar results when the teachers' data was compared with the results of audiometric screening test results, as was done in the above two studies. when the teachers' data was compared with the results of rescreening and tympanometry followed by otoscopy, agreement doubled and teachers were found to be able to identify 47% of the children who had a hearing loss (haggard & hughes, 1991). savary and ferron (1982) found that out of 265 children who were identified by teachers as having 'school pathology', 35% had audiographic abnormalities and 45% had ear conditions requiring otolaryngologic management. in south africa it is becoming increasingly common for young children to be placed in pre-schools while both parents or single parents attend work. pre-school teachers see the children in their classes on a regular, daily basis and often spend more time with them than any other individual. this study suggests utilizing teachers in the hearing screening process for a number of reasons: they are already available; no additional salaries need to be paid; they have some knowledge about speech and language in children; they are easier to educate than parents as there are fewer of them, and they may be easier to reach; they can educate parents about hearing problems; they see the children on a regular basis; and they can. administer the questionnaire a number of times throughout the year. in addition to this, they may be able to pick up subtle changes in children's behaviour because they see them so frequently. this study aims to determine teacher accuracy in the identification of pre-school pupils with hearing loss by means of a questionnaire. teacher identification will be compared firstly to audiometric only and then to combined audiometric and tympanometric screening results. method subjects subject selection criteria teachers , the teachers selected as subjects for the study had to be those who were the class teachers of the pupils involved in a hearing screening practical carried out by second year speech and hearing therapy students from the university of the witwatersrand. this practical took place from september 1996 to april 1997. pupils the pupils selected had to fall into the three year to six year eleven month age range and had to be involved in the second year hearing screening practical. description of subjects teachers thirteen teachers from three nursery schools and two childcare centres were involved in the study. table 1 gives a description of the teachers' qualifications and their number of years of teaching experience. there was much diversity in the qualifications, but the most common qualification was a diploma in pre-primary education. the years of teaching experience ranged from one year to nineteen years with the mean being ten years experience. pupils in total, questionnaires were completed for 210 children. of these, 100 pupils were not used. this was due to the following: many did not have their hearing screened as they were either absent or did not return their consent forms; the birth dates for some of the children were not filled in and thus their ages were not known; two of the pupils had joined the nursery school two weeks before the screening and the teacher did not feel that she had enough knowledge to complete the questionnaire for them; and one of the children for whom the questionnaire was completed was younger than three years. the mean age of the final sample was 4 years 7 months and the ages ranged from 3 years to 6 years 8 months with the most common age range being 3 years 6 months to 3 years 11 months. refer to figure 1 for the pupil-age distribution. table 2 gives a description of the teacher-pupil distribution across the pre-schools according to the 110 pupils in the sample. the average ratio of teachers to pupils involved in the study is 1:8.5. equipment and materials equipment portable audiometers these included two ιηίβ^οομβίίοβ as7 screening audiometers with peltor "type a earphones, two eckstein brothers 390mb audiometers with maico 78711 earphones, the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) teacher accuracy in the identification of pre-school pupils with hearing loss materials 27 one maico ma20 audiometer with tdh 39p earphones, and one maico ma 40 audiometer with tdh 39 earphones. all of the audiometers were calibrated in june 1996. immittance machines three interacoustics at22 impedance audiometers were used. these were calibrated in june 1996. otoscopes welch allyn klinic otoscopes with detachable speculae were used. table 1: teacher qualifications and teaching experience teacher qualifications years experience 1 n3; presently studying n4 to n6 through the johannesburg college of education 1 2 national higher certificate in child care 8 3 degree in education from the university of liverpool 12 4 diploma in pre-primary education 14 5 diploma in pre-primary education; diploma in child care 5 6 1 year child care qualification 4 7 senior primary higher education diploma 7 8 . ' / / higher education diploma in pre-primary and primary; degree in pre-primary education 15 9 diploma in junior primary education | 14 10 bachelor of arts degree 4 11 diploma in nursing psychology 13 12 lmc training in london 14 13 ami training in france 19 a questionnaire was used to gain information from the teachers about the school performance, speech, language and hearing of each of their pupils (see appendix a). the questionnaire was especially brief because the teachers were required to complete the questions for all of their pupils. it was kept as simple as possible requiring only a cross or tick or one word response for each question. an example was given of how the questionnaire should be completed so that the teachers knew what responses were expected and where they were meant to respond to each question. the questions in the questionnaire were divided into seven main groups. i. identifying information: i.e., 'name' and 'date of birth' ii. school performance: e.g., 'concentration and attention' (roeser & downs, 1988) iii. hearing information: e.g., 'hears and answers when first called' (roeser & downs, 1988; american speech and language foundation pamphlet, 1986; masland, 1970) and 'turns head to where sound came from' (swart, 1996b) speech and language information: e.g., 'says all sounds other than r, 1, th, s' (roeser & downs, 1988; american speech and language foundation pamphlet, 1986) medical information: e.g., 'has had ear infections' (roeser & downs, 1988) and 'child has allergies' (swart, 1996; haggard & hughes, 1991) iv. v. 35 3-3.5 3.6-3.11 4-4.5 4.6-4.11 5-5.5 5.6-5.11 6-6.5 6.6-6.11 age groups figure 1: pupil age distribution in years and months. table 2: teacher pupil distribution across pre-schools according to the specific sample used in this study school aletta sutton educare centre greenhouse child care centre hansel and gretel nursery school hug-a-bug nursery school melville montessori nursery school total no. teachers 4 2 2 3 2 13 no. pupils 1 17 15 14 38 26 110 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif ir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 nicole chambers & on anderson vi. other problems or any other important information vii. main question: i.e., 'do you think the child would pass/fail a hearing test?' the questions in groups ii. v. were included in the questionnaire as the information obtained from them could alert one to the possible presence of a hearing loss and might prove useful to teachers in identifying potential hearing loss. procedure teachers were given questionnaires which they were required to complete to the best of their ability. no form of intervention or training was given in the use of the questionnaire or on speech, language and hearing. the questionnaires were collected on the day of the hearing screening. the second year speech and hearing therapy screened the hearing of each pupil; provided they had parental permission. the students were supervised by staff members from the university of the witwatersrand department of speech pathology and audiology. the hearing screening included otoscopy, tympanometry, and pure tone audiometry via air conduction at 1000 hz, 2000 hz and 4000 hz at 20 db calculated using biological calibration (katz, 1994). otoscopy was used to check for perforated tympanic membranes, impacted wax or foreign bodies in the external ear canal to ensure appropriate testing and ent management. criteria for failure were a type β or type c tympanometry result (northern & downs, 1991) and / or failure to respond to any two frequencies during pure tone testing (irrespective of whether they occurred in one or two ears). pure tone testing was performed in the quietest available room at each of the pre-schools. the windows and doors of the rooms were closed in order to reduce background noise. however, some background noise still existed. biological calibration was used in an attempt to account for the effects of the noise but it must be noted that the lack of a soundproof environment may have had an affect on the accuracy of the pure tone results. results pupils were classified into two groups: those who passed the screening and those who failed the screening. they were first classified on the basis of audiometric screening results and then on the basis of combined audiometric and tympanometric screening results. figure 2 is a comparison of the teacher identification results and the audiometaudiometric identification no. pupils fail no. pupils pass * a ο th 'j3 ϊ ee ss ο y is no. pupils fail 1 8 ce -s s fi φ ό i—h no. pupils pass 5 96 figure 2: error matrix comparing teacher identification with audiometric identification ric screening results. the teachers identified 8.2% of the total sample as having a hearing problem, whereas only 5.5% of the pupils tested failed the audiometric screening. furthermore, there was agreement between teacher ' identification and audiometric identification on only 1% of the total sample. the mcnemar chi square was computed and found to be 0,31 for 1 degree of freedom, and indicated that no significant difference existed between teacher identification and audiometric identification (mccall & kagan, 1994). the confidence interval for proportion was calculated and indicated that the probability is 0.95 that the proportion of pupils who were classified differently by teacher identification and audiometric identification, lies between 5.78% and 17.84% of total cases with the best estimate being at 11.81% (mccall & kagan, 1994). figure 3 compares teacher identifications with combined audiometric and tympanometric screening results. the agreement between the two procedures has increased to 3.7%; it has tripled with regard to hearing loss. the mcnemar chi square was computed and found to be 12.03, which is significant beyond 0.01 for 1 degree of freedom (moall & kagan, 1994). this indicates that there was a significant difference between teacher identification and combined audiometric and tympanometric identification. the confidence interval for proportion was calculated and indicated that the probability is 0.95 that the proportion of pupils who were classified differently by teacher identification and combined identification, lies between 18.97% and 35.57% of total cases (m'call & kagan, 1994). descriptive analysis of the teacher responses on the questionnaire was carried out. in the cases where the teachers correctly identified the pupils who failed the hearing screening, the following responses were noted (from most to least common): poor grammar, vocabulary and syntax; poor concentration and attention; lack of understanding of most speech; inability to pronounce 'r', ί', 'th' or's'; presence of allergies; below average class performance; loud speaking voice; and second language english. perhaps these factors alerted the teachers to the presence of hearing problems. 1 in the cases where the teachers failed to identify the pupils who failed the hearing screening, two factors were noted by the teachers but did not seem to alert them to the presence of a hearing problem: poor classroom interaction, and an inability to hear and answer when first called. it is somewhat surprising that the latter factor did not alert the teachers to a hearing problem, as it woul'd audiometric and tympanometric identification no. pupils fail no. pupils pass a ο th φ ce rc ο y ίΰ no. pupils fail 4 > ce -13 φ pi a a> x) l—h no. pupils pass 25 76 figure 3: error matrix comparing teacher identification with combined audiometric and tympanometric screening results the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) teacher accuracy in the identification of pre-school pupils with hearing loss 29 appear to be an obvious indicator of a hearing problem. for the teachers who correctly identified one or more pupils who failed the hearing screening, years of teaching experience ranged from thirteen to nineteen years with a mean of fifteen years, and qualifications included a diploma in nursing psychology, lmc training in london, and ami training in france. for those teachers who did not correctly identify any pupils, years of teaching experience ranged from one to fifteen years with a mean of eight years. these results would suggest that successful teacher identification is associated with greater years in teaching experience. this did not hold true for all cases, however, as four of the teachers who did not identify any pupils, had between twelve and fifteen years teaching experience. discussion the results of this study suggest that without prior education or training in the use of a questionnaire, preschool teachers are not accurate in identifying pupils with hearing problems, and should not be used to detect hearing losses in the pre-school population. findings tend to support those of kodman (1956) who found that teachers could correctly identify one out of six pupils with hearing losses, as determined by audiometric testing. results are also similar to those obtained by curry (1950) who found that in comparison to audiometric testing, teachers could identify one out of four hearing loss cases. results obtained by nodar (1978) were more favourable than the results obtained in this study. he found that in comparison to audiometric testing teachers identified one out of three hearing loss cases, while in comparison to combined audiometric and tympanometric testing they identified one out of two. perhaps these differences in results are due to the fact that the pupils in nodar's study were in the 5-12 year age range, while the pupils involved in this study were between 3 years and 6 years 11 months of age. results found, both in this study and in nodar's study, support the view that there is a decrease in hearing problems with an increase in age, so one would expect a much higher incidence of jhearing problems in this study xthan in nodar's. findings in fact do support this as 4% of the total sample were found to have hearing problems in nodar's study, while 26% were identified with hearing problems in this study. one would expect to find a higher incidence of otitis media iii this study than in nodar's study because of the younger age range of the sample. otitis media often leads to mild hearing losses, which may be more difficult for teachers to identify. this could account for their poorer performance in identification which was noted in this study. perhaps teachers find it easier to identify hearing problems in older pupils due to differences in the behaviour of older children as compared to younger children. the teachers reported having difficulty determining whether some of their pupils had hearing problems, or were simply having difficulties due to the fact that they were second language english .speakers. for example, they were unsure whether some of their pupils with poor language, poor concentration, and inappropriate responses were hearing-impaired or simply experiencing these problems due to a poor understanding of the english language. the majority of the children in this study were second or third language english speakers and this could have had a negative effect on teacher identification. there were also methodological differences that might have accounted for the difference in results obtained between nodar's study and this study. nodar (1978) used a 25 dbhl audiometric screening level, while this study used a 20 dbhl level. nodar (1978) also included the 250 hz, 500 hz and 6000 hz frequencies in his audiometric screening, while these frequencies were excluded from the audiometric screening in this study. perhaps these variations also contributed to the differences in results. the results of this study also differed from those of savary and ferron (1982). they found 35% of pupils identified by teachers had audiographic abnormalities while 45% had otolaryngologic disease processes. once again methodological differences existed between the two studies. savary and ferron (1982) instructed teachers to identify any pupils who experienced a drop in grades; or who developed behavioural, language or learning problems. perhaps this extra instruction led to better teacher performance. because savary and ferron (1982) believe that hearing impairment is a hearing disorder at any level of severity, their criteria for failure included any air-bone gap of 10 db or more on three conversational frequencies. this does not follow the screening criteria recommended by katz (1994). it is generally considered that hearing threshold levels of above 25 db constitute hearing loss; that an air-bone gap of 10 db or less is not clinically significant (hodgson, 1985); and that bone conduction testing is not included as part of hearing screening procedures. because savary and ferron (1982) used stricter criteria than these, perhaps it is possible that their audiological testing identified some false positive cases. if this is the case then it is possible that some of the teacher identifications were also false positives, and that teacher identification was not actually as good as reported. if the screening criteria recommended by katz (1994) were used in the savary and ferron (1982) study, the results may have been similar to those obtained in this study. the teachers from the pre-schools involved in this study demonstrated a willingness and eagerness to learn more about speech, language and hearing in their pupils. they responded favourably to offers of a workshop to their preschool staff, covering these areas. in addition to this, the many other advantages of utilizing teachers in the screening process still remain: they are already available and see their pupils on a regular basis;.no additional salaries need to be paid; and they have some knowledge about speech and language in children. perhaps future research could repeat this study with the inclusion of teacher education and training in the use of a questionnaire in identifying hearing problems in pupils. teachers coiild be given guidelines on when to fail pupils. for example, if the child displays one or more of a given list of behaviours and physical symptoms that may indicate a hearing problem (roeser & downs, 1988). the educational background of audiologists equips them to assume the role of'educators' and to be responsible for the effective training of teachers. this brings us back to the problem of having too few audiologists in south africa to deal with all the demands of the population. to oveicome this problem perhaps a course on hearing education and training in the use of a questionnaire, could be included in the final year syllabus of teaching students. this would ensure that all teaching students are reached and that they are educated and trained in the identification of hearing problems. it would be cost-effective, time-effective, and would require fewer audiologists to act as 'edudie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 n i c o l e c h a m b e r s & i l o n a a n d e r s o n cators'. it could also serve to h e i g h t e n g e n e r a l a w a r e n e s s of hearing-impairment. b e a r i n g the r e s u l t s o f this study in m i n d it m i g h t be useful to i n c r e a s e the training a n d education p e r i o d for those teachers w i t h fewer years teachi n g e x p e r i e n c e . c o n c l u s i o n this s t u d y l o o k e d at teacher a c c u r a c y in the identification of p u p i l s w i t h h e a r i n g loss c o m p a r e d to audiometric s c r e e n i n g r e s u l t s , a n d c o m b i n e d a u d i o m e t r i c a n d t y m p a n o m e t r i c s c r e e n i n g results. f i n d i n g s from the study r e v e a l e d t h a t t e a c h e r s identified only one o u t o f six pupils w h o failed a u d i o m e t r i c screening, a n d one o u t of seven p u p i l s w h o f a i l e d c o m b i n e d a u d i o m e t r i c a n d t y m p a n o m e t r i c s c r e e n i n g . a g r e e m e n t b e t w e e n t e a c h e r identification a n d a u d i o m e t r i c identification w a s only o n 1% o f the total s a m p l e while there w a s a g r e e m e n t b e t w e e n teacher identification a n d c o m b i n e d identification o n 3.7% of the total s a m p l e . it w a s c o n c l u d e d t h a t teachers w e r e n o t a c c u r a t e i n identifying pupils w i t h h e a r i n g loss a n d thus should n o t b e u s e d to detect hearing p r o b l e m s in their pupils, u n l e s s t h e y are g i v e n prior e d u c a t i o n , a n d training in the u s e o f a questionnaire. t h u s it a p p e a r s i m p e r a tive t h a t i n o r d e r to o v e r c o m e v a r i o u s s c r e e n i n g p r o b l e m s b y utilizing t e a c h e r s i n the s c r e e n i n g process, a period o f teacher e d u c a t i o n a n d training i n the use of a screening q u e s t i o n n a i r e is essential. a c k n o w l e d g e m e n t s w e w o u l d l i k e to t h a n k p e t e r f r i d j h o n a n d m i k e greyling for their assistance w i t h the statistical analysis of this study. r e f e r e n c e s american speech and hearing foundation and psi iota xi sorority. (1986). find your child's speech and hearing age. maryland : nahsa. child health policy group. (1996). workshop on screening for developmental disability in the pre-school population university of cape town, child health unit. curry, e.t. (1950). the efficiency of teacher referrals in a school hearing testing program. j. speech hear. disord., 15 211-214 davis, j.m., elfenbein, j., schum, r. & bentler, r.a. (1986)! effects of mild and moderate hearing impairments on language, educational, and psychosocial behaviour of children j. speech hear. disord., 51 53-62. haggard, m. & hughes, e. (1991). screening children's hearing: a review of the literature and the implications of otitis media london : hmso. hodgson, w.r. (1985). basic audiologic evaluation. florida : robert e. krieger publishing company. katz, j. (1994). handbook of clinical audiology (4th ed.). baltimore : williams and wilkins. kodman, f. (1956). identification of hearing loss by the classroom teacher. laryngoscope, 66 1346-1349. masland, m.w. (1970). speech and hearing checklist. volta review. washington : alexander graham bell association for the deaf. mccall, r.b. & kagan, j. (1994). fundamental statistics for behavioural sciences (6th ed.). usa : harcourt brace college publishers. menchor, g.t. & m'culloch, b.f. (1970). auditory screening of kindergarten children vasc. j. speech hear. disord., 35 241247. nih consensus statement on early. identification of hearing impairment in infants and young children. (1993). 11(1) 3nodar, r.h. (1978). teacher identification of elementary school children with hearing loss. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 9 24-28. northern, j.l. & downs, m.p. (1991). hearing in children (3rd ed.). baltimore : williams and wilkins. roeser, r.j. & downs, m.p. (1988j. screening for hearing loss and middle ear disorders: auditory disorders in school children (2nd ed.). new york : thieme medical publishers inc. savary, p. & ferron, p. (1982). screening of hearing disorders in schoolchildren. american journal of otolaryngology., 3 388391. south african medical and dental council. (1997). telephonic information. swart, s. (1995λ deafsa: policy document on early identification and ear care. unpublished. swart, s. (1996a;. hearing loss in infancy and childhood: early identification and intervention. unpublished. swart, s. (1996b). hearing loss in infancy and childhood: early identification and intervention. health and hygiene, (supplement) september / october. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 25 departmental evaluation: speech therapy and audiology, an academic department aimed at teaching a profession s r hugo department of communication pathology university of pretoria w μ j hugo director: graduate school of business leadership, unisa abstract in a recently established policy guideline of the department of national education it was clearly indicated that programmes and departments at tertiary education institutions should institute evaluation procedures as a matter of urgency. this has become necessary to provide a basis for decision making and policy formation, to accredit programmes, to monitor expenditure of public funds and to improve educational material. it is the aim of this paper to develop an evaluation model, based on accepted education principles, for evaluating the actual performance of a department of speech therapy and audiology. a basic management model is used and specific functional indications are given of the steps that should be taken in evaluating a department. the model is a comprehensive one which can easily be adapted for use in other departments. opsomming in 'n belangwekkende publikasie van die departement van nasionale opvoeding is aangedui dat dit tans gebiedend noodsaaklik geword het dat departemente verbonde aan tersiire opleidingsinrigtings dringend moet beplan om evalueringsprogramme te loots. die redes hiervoor is dat evaluering basisinligting verskafvir beleidsbesluite, akkreditering van programme moontlik maak, besteding van fondse monitor en akademiese opleidingsmateriaal en -stelsels verbeter. dit is die doel van hierdie artikel om 'n evalueringsmodel, gegrond op erkende opvoedkundige beginsels, te ontwikkel virgebruik in die bepaling van waarde van 'n departement vir spraakheelkunde en oudiologie. 'n bestuursmodel is geselekteer en spesifieke aanduidings is verskafvan die strategies, stappe en norms wat gebruik kan word in departementele evaluering. die model is 'n omvattende een wat suksesvol aangepas kan word vir gebruik deur ander departemente. "due to the complex nature of human communication and its disorders, it is essential that information of an academic, research and scientific nature is continually developed and evaluated. a university provides the optimal environment for constant academic validation of current professional training." feldman 1981, ρ 942 1 introduction evaluation, or the need to determine the worth and merit of whatever is being evaluated, is a well-known concept. there is ample evidence that evaluation in education was an accepted practice even prior to 1920. from the literature, it is equally clear that the debate is still raging on how evaluation should be conducted and what role it should fulfil in different spheres. the emergence of educational evaluation in terms of student outcomes and institutional effectiveness has been a significant trend in higher education in the past few years (nichols, 1989, ρ vii). in the united states of america, for example, standards are established and enforced to ensure institutional quality. to maintain accredited status, institutions must be evaluated periodically to demonstrate achievement of standards and to document ongoing efforts for institutional improvement (middleton et al., 1989, ρ 15). in europe too, there is growing emphasis on the philosophy of value for money, and funding for higher education is therefore being linked directly to performance (goedebuure et al., 1990, ρ 15). institutions for higher education throughout the world increasingly accept the fact that they are accountable to political authorities and to the communities they serve. establishing evaluation programmes at institutional, departmental and programme levels in higher education serves the dual purpose of proving quality of performance to stakeholders, while at the same time protecting the traditional autonomy of tertiary education institutions. in south africa, education authorities clearly indicated in a policy statement that academic departments are expected to institute evaluation procedures aimed at internal evaluation and to plan for external evaluation according to prescribed guidelines (nasop 02-129 87/10). in spite of the contemporary urgency of evaluation at tertiary level, no formal and generally acceptable evaluae suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 s.r. hugo & w.m.j. hugo tion model has been developed that could be used to provide quantitative information on the actual performance of departments in general. wolf (1990, ρ 19) reports that there are no less than fourteen different evaluation models and five different approaches. there is also little agreement or data to support the efficacy of one model or approach over another. evidently the fundamental problem is that the various models are built on differing and often conflicting conceptions and definitions of evaluation (worthen & sanders 1987, ρ 44). in view of the above, the following problem statement can be formulated: what are the principles involved in structuring an evaluation model for an academic department engaged in a professional programme aimed at developing speech therapists and audiologists? 2 research objectives and design the objective of the study is to develop an evaluation model, based on accepted educational principles, for evaluating the actual performance of a department of speech therapy and audiology in south africa, in order to address the specific needs of this unique and divers context. to attain this objective, a research plan comprising of two consecutive stages was devised: • establishing the underlying principles of evaluating programmes and departments in tertiary education institutions by means of a comprehensive literature study • developing a proposed model for the evaluation of a department of speech therapy and audiology. 3 conceptual and contextual issues of evaluation 3.1 terminology and concepts as previously indicated, various approaches and definitions of evaluation may lead to fundamental differences in educational evaluation. the term evaluation for example, may be interpreted to mean measurement, appraisal, assessment, professional judgment, scientific enquiry, a political activity or an act of collecting and providing information (worthen & sanders 1987, ρ 22). in this study, evaluation will be interpreted as the act of rendering judgment to determine worth or merit that is: ... the making of judgments about the worth and effectiveness of educational intentions, processes and outcomes; about the relationships between these; and about the resource, planning and implementation frameworks for such ventures. (adelman & alexander, 1982, ρ 5.) because of differences in interpretation and the divergent μββ of evaluation processes in various institutions and also in different countries, the meaning attached to evaluation concepts and procedures may be utterly confusing. nisbet (1988, ρ 4) suggests a cognitive map to assist in establishing a common understanding of concepts and their role in evaluation. this is represented in figure 1. only the most important (and perhaps most controversial) concepts referred to in this figure will be dealt with here. the controversy about the formative or summative nature of evaluation has raged for many decades. in essence, the question is whether evaluation should provide immediate feedback of information useful to programme developers who would use the information for revision during the development process (formative evaluation). contrary to this point of view, summative evaluation is conducted at the end of the programme and is aimed at providing judgment about the value of a programme. since the evaluation of an academic department is the subject of this study and because academic departments, like academic programmes are forever changing and in a state of development, formative evaluation is of crucial importance. however, it is equally important to provide feedback to authorities about the actual performance of an academic department at particular times (at the end of an academic or financial year or review cycle, for example). hence evaluation in the context of this study must also perform a summative role. linked to the issue of formative and summative evaluation is the question of internal or external evaluation. because of the immediateness of formative evaluation, information provided by this type of evaluation is valuable to departmental managers and often used to effect improvements. this type of evaluation is therefore frequently undertaken internally with internal evaluation objectives in mind. summative evaluation, however, can be used for accountability and is often undertaken by external evaluators commissioned by some external audience or decision maker. as an extension of the argument that formative as well as summative evaluation play an essential role in departmental evaluation, it is evident that in evaluating the academic department, both internal and external evaluation are essential. the quantitative or qualitative base of evaluation is another conceptual issue referred to in the cognitive map depicted in figure 1. qualitative inquiry focuses on processes rather than outcomes and uses an inductive approach to data analysis while quantitative inquiry focuses on numbers by emphasizing standardization, precision, and objectivity (worthen & sanders, 1987, ρ 51). j however, in departmental evaluation quantitative and qualitative methods can be regarded as compatible and complementary approaches. worthen and sanders (1987, ρ 53) support this premise and point out that the trend in evaluation today is towards methodological pluralism. the concepts of accountability and professionalism are of particular importance in the context of evaluation (see figure 1). educational accountability is the responsibility for identifying and measuring educational outcomes and using the information about these outcomes for decision making (kogan, 1986, ρ 75). accountability has three major dimensions in education. moral accountability is related to the fact that academic institutions are answerable to their clients. this implies that if resources are spent on educational programmes, it is necessary to determine whether the programmes (or departments) achieve what they are designed to achieve (lee & sampson, 1990, ρ 157). evaluation of performance outcomes provides proof of academic quality and it is important to note that funding for higher education is increasingly linked to the quality of this performance (goedegebuure et al., 1990: 15). professional accountability refers to the fact that academicians are answerable to themselves and to their colleagues in the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) departmental evaluation: speech therapy and audiology 27 η top-down hard data appraisal external evaluation outward-looking accountability teacher as trained practitioner assessment testing control performance indicators product summative judgement evaluation η % % % formative flexibility process understanding insights growth management objectives planning rational x teacher as reflective professional % teacher involvement commitment consensus building internal evaluation self-evaluation grass-roots professionalism inwnrd-iooking figure 1: evaluation a cognitive map. academy. professional accountability is therefore fundamental to self-evaluation and peer evaluation. peer evaluation is indeed the oldest and most fundamental of all evaluation principles in higher education (kogan, 1989, ρ 118). finally, educational accountability also encompasses contractual accountability. academic departments are therefore answerable to management in higher education as well as to political authorities. 3.2 purpose and objectives of departmental evaluation evidently the first step in any evaluation procedure is to determine the purpose and objectives of the evaluation process (worthen & slanders, 1987, ρ 147). this is not only dictated by logic but is vitally important because educational evaluation is always undertaken within a particular context. important contextual parameters can be determined by clarifying beforehand issues such as why evaluation is being undertaken, who the main audiences are, to what use evaluation findings may be put, what method of enquiry should be used and who should do the evaluation (house, 1986, ρ 16). an analysis indicates that the purposes and objectives of departmental evaluation can be subdivided into three major categories. • improvement (organisational learning) a major objective of departmental evaluation is to generate useful information for the improvement of departmental activities: information to management of the department for decision making and to do planning. seldin (1988, ρ 24) points out that there is no better reason to evaluate than to improve performance. evaluation data assist the faltering, motivate the tired and encourage the indecisive. supplementary (but related) to this, is the evaluation objective of incorporating into the operation of the academic department an ongoing concern for self-study and self-improvement (kells, 1989 ρ 100). • accreditation in this category, the objective of evaluation is to assess the extent to which accreditation standards are being met. it also provides useful written material for the evaluation team and formal reports for the accreditation authorities. • accountability as previously indicated, a major objective of evaluation is to provide proof that resources are used optimally (contractual and moral accountability). an additional objective is to ascertain the quality of higher education provided by the academic department (professional accountability). finally, the objective of the evaluation process should be to provide a clear statement of the relationships between the goal and objectives of the academic department and the mission statement of the institution (contractual accountability). the importance of the conceptual and contextual issues discussed in the above paragraphs gains additional perspective for departmental evaluation when viewed against the background of specific concerns relevant for academic training for professional programmes. 3.3 specific concerns relevant to evaluating education programmes for the professions the central feature that distinguishes a professional academic programme from general university courses can die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 s.r. hugo & w.m.j. hugo be found in the definition of a profession. according to hughes (1963, in goodlad, 1985, ρ 7) the central feature of all professions is the motto "credat emptor" (let the buyer trust). thus is the professional relation distinguished from markets in which the rule is "caveat emptor" (let the buyer beware). to develop an individual that embodies this code of conduct implies the incorporation of very specific academic aims, strategies and procedures in the education programme. the one unchanging feature of professional training (and perhaps the main factor that distinguishes professional academic training from other university programmes) is unceasing movement towards new levels of performance. in this energizing process some of the following characteristics are observable: • concern with mission / function clarification • mastery of theoretical knowledge • capacity to solve problems • credentially • public acceptance • ethical practices (nowlen, 1988). this feature is usually utilized in the evaluation done by professional training bodies. these bodies play an important role in establishing minimum standards for education and training, in controlling professional ethics and even in structuring university programmes. this may, on the one hand, be negative because it can function in direct opposition to the autonomy of academic institutions. on the other hand the professional bodies can contribute to the objective determination of worth of a specific department because of their role as independent external observer of actual performance. there are moreover specific considerations appropriate to the evaluation of an academic department concerned with programmes for speech therapy and audiology. firstly, academic departments for speech therapy and audiology in the rsa have the general characteristics of all academic departments in a developing country. their main characteristic is the fact that up to now the departments have failed to establish sustainable capacity for monitoring and evaluation. in consequence there is a lack of information on educational outcomes and costs. in an era of constrained resources for education development this implies that the effects of investments are difficult to gauge and lessons of experience difficult to accumulate (middleton, et al., 1989, ρ 1). secondly, on a ground roots level, evaluation of departments for speech therapy and audiology must take certain practical characteristics into consideration. these include: π a low student-lecturer ratio and in conjunction with this a low student enrolment figure attributed to the policy of individual training and practical limitations; high grades of students and a low failure rate because of student selection practised by most universities; π the difficulty in evaluating practical training of students where qualitative rather than quantitative measures are employed; • the fact that practical training is dependent on the availability and cooperation of outside agencies like hospitals, schools and clinics; • the influence on programmes by systems independent of the academic institutions, such as the national health policy, the professional board for speech therapy and audiology and the south african medical en dental council. 4 a model for the departmental evaluation process 4.1 conceptual basis a classification by worthen and sanders (1987, ρ 60) of different approaches to evaluation identifies six categories: π objective oriented approaches π management oriented approaches • consumer oriented approaches • expertise oriented approaches • adversary oriented approaches • naturalisticand participant-oriented approaches in analysing each of the above categories it became immediately obvious that the management approach would be a valuable basis for evaluation of a professional academic department for speech therapy and audiology. there are three compelling reasons for this preference. the management approach to education which regards education as essentially an instrumental activity designed to bring about the achievement of specifiable and uncontroversial educational goals is becoming increasingly dominant (mortimore & stone, 1990, ρ 69). this is especially true of tertiary institutions in general and universities in particular. the choice of the management approach for departmental evaluation is therefore congruent with a general trend. the second reason is that any department of speech therapy and audiology is today, with the general movement at universities towards accreditation, reduced funding and unavailability of qualified academic staff (strydom et al., 1991), involved in a struggle for survival. a management approach towards evaluation will provide valuable information for decision making (and decision makers) in this endeavour. lastly the management approach, if correctly understood and used in context, includes many of the characteristics and principles of the other six approaches towards evaluation in education. it is therefore a comprehensive basis for departmental evaluation. i the questions now are: what is the management approach to evaluation and how.can it be utilized by a department of speech therapy and audiology? in their analysis, worthen and sanders (1987, ρ 77) state that the management approach to evaluation is meant to serve decision makers. they continue to emphasise that, by highlighting different levels of decision makers (levels of management), this approach clarifies who will use evaluation results, how they will use them and what aspects of the system they will be making decisions about. in the normal management control procedure in an organisation information about actual performance is channelled to operational management, middle management and top management in accordance with the nature of the information and the control process. this is conceptually identical to the principal characteristics of educathe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) departmental evaluation: speech therapy and audiology 29 1. internal departmental self-study 2. external review or site visit by experts 3. review of reports • audience operational management • audience middle management • audience top management • formative evaluation • formative / summative evaluation • summative evaluation • aimed at improvement • improvement and reporting • review reports generated by selfstudy and peer group • departmental management • peer group of professionals • faculty / institutional management / political authorities • professionally | accountable • professionally / contractually accountable • contractual accountable figure 2: management approach to departmental evaluation. tional evaluation referred to in paragraph 3.1.and can be illustrated in figure 2. firstly, according to the improvement purpose of evaluation, information is provided to departmental management to continually effect improvement. this is clearly formative evaluation aimed at enabling departmental management to be professionally accountable. secondly, in accordance with the accreditation purpose, information about the department and the actual performance of it's activities is forwarded to some higher authority (faculty management level or even (institutional management level). this evaluation is clearly of a summative nature although it may contain some formative elements. evaluation information at this level enables departmental management to be contractuallyj accountable. thirdly, in order to answer to moral accountability, evaluation information is provided to the highest management levels in tertiary education, the political authority that controls funding and also to other stakeholders such as professional boards and statutory controlling bodies. the control hierarchy typical of ordinary management control procedures is therefore also embedded in the basic principles of educational evaluation. the implications of the preceding arguments for evaluating a department of speech therapy and audiology using the management approach, are in essence that a threetier evaluation hierarchy with the following characteristics should be constructed (adapted from kells, 1989, ρ 99): π an internal departmental self study prepared by the professionals concerned, using an evaluation model with a management approach; π an external review or site visit by experts in speech therapy and audiology (peer evaluation) using the evaluation data generated by the internal study and other suitable evaluation procedures. this can be used in conjunction with a report from the professional board for speech therapy and audiology, but should not be based on such an evaluation alone; • a review of the internal departmental report(s) and the reports of the external peer evaluation by a professional group in the institution (faculty management or institutional management) and by the controlling board of the profession. 4.2 proposed model the evaluation model proposed in this section is a conceptual model based on the general principles of educational evaluation and on guidelines for departmental evaluation discussed in previous paragraphs. the model is illustrated in figure 3, and in broad terms follows six logical steps. 4.2.1 preplanning for evaluation the first step for departmental management is to identify clearly the need for evaluation why the department should be evaluated. this decision can only be finalised once the persons and organisations who are likely to be interested in the evaluation results have been identified. information generated by evaluating the department of speech therapy and audiology is aimed at a three-tier audience departmental management; an external review board of experts; and at top management, at institutional and political levels. in general the reasons for evaluating an academic department can include any of the following (worthen & sanders, 1987, ρ 8): • improving student development and performance • enhancing educator qualifications and performance • improving the departmental organisational structure • upgrading textbooks and other curriculum materials and products π determining whether to continue, modify, expand or terminate an existing programme • reviewing and improving curriculum design and processes. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 s.r. hugo & w.m.j. hugo pre-planning • need' . • audience • objectives" * / ν <'«*<%; , processloesignf activity areas scheduling p e r s o n n e l ϊ designing performance indicators admin • financing • workload • user satisfaction process γ "-"-^.outcomes ί management • planning • organising •control . r if (fj »,„<•, >> !j i ; r ii -may f & proficiency • teaching • research •community service efficiency • manpower •finances •facilities comparison objective information gathering assessment o f actual performance reporting figure 3: evaluation model. this step also involves demarcating exactly what should be evaluated (for example, administration, programmes, organisational structures or research outputs) and identifying the purposes and objectives of the evaluation. finally, the pre-planning phase should also include a survey of the resources available for evaluation. 4.2.2 evaluation process design drafting a plan of action for departmental evaluation is the main objective of this step. based on the demarcation exercise in the first step and on the purpose and objectives of the evaluation, major departmental activity areas to be evaluated must be identified. , ^ figure 3 indicates that departmental evaluation for speech therapy and audiology can be subdivided into two major but interrelated entities: process evaluation and outcomes evaluation (adapted from worthen & sanders, 1987, ρ 77 and house 1986, ρ 17). on the process side, administrative evaluation is aimed at how well the administrative support activities of the the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) departmental evaluation: speech therapy and audiology 31 department function. in addition, the evaluation is also aimed at determining how well the department is being managed. process evaluation provides an overview of the ability of the department to function dynamically and flexibly and of its capacity to survive. in the outcomes section there are two major subdivisions or components. evaluation procedures aimed at establishing how well the department is doing what it is supposed to do, i.e. success in attaining its objectives (proficiency also referred to as effectiveness) is one component. the second is the efficiency component, and it refers to how well the department is utilising the resources at its disposal. figure 3 also shows that the major objectives of the department that must be realised are in the activity areas of education, research and community service. the most important resources at the disposal of the department are manpower, finances and facilities. 4.2.3 planning and designing performance indicators for evaluation establishing performance measures and the concomitant norms, involves, in effect, designing yardsticks for the practical evaluation of departmental performance. a performance measure represents an integrated group or category of related departmental characteristics or activities that jointly provide a picture of a specific departmental outcome or result. evaluation norms are a series of measurement techniques whereby quantitative values and/or relationships can be determined for specific departmental activities, several of which jointly depict departmental performance in terms of a performance measure (hugo & van rooyen, 1990, ρ 295). it is essential that a large number of measures and norms be determined for each major activity area identified in step 2. here follow some measures and norms that may serve as examples. it isjhowever necessary that every department should develop jits own specific measures and norms based on internal characteristics and in conjunction with the aims of the department. 4.2.3.1 measures and norms for process evaluation • administration j measures and norms for administrative activities must be based on a comprehensiye description of the administrative task. measures and norms evaluating both proficiency and efficiency are essential. the following are examples (adapted from wirgin & braskamp, 1987, ρ 29). 1. financial consideration: 3. user satisfaction: administrative services: total cost ratio annual administrative budget cost of typing = total departmental budget annual cost of typing staff number of pages of study material produced 2. administrative workload: spread number of assignments processed a month average number of assignments processed per month lead times = average waiting time (hours or days) for typing, photocopying any month travel arrangements average waiting time for same tasks previous month number of errors in travel arrangements total number of requests for travel arrangements per time period • management evaluation of management attempts to obtain a picture of how thoroughly the management elements of planning, organising, coordinating and controlling are implemented. at managerial level the management-elements are therefore used as performance measures. bearing in mind that the managerial process is an interactive process between the different elements of management, it is obviously not possible (nor desirable) to isolate the contribution to performance outputs of any particular element of management in the evaluation process. the evaluation of management performance is therefore largely conducted on a subjective basis with the aid of an evaluation sheet or questionnaire. 4.2.3.2 measures and norms for outcomes 4.2.3.2.1 proficiency proficiency measures and norms are aimed at determining how successful a department of speech therapy and audiology is in attaining its goals. the most important of these goals are in the three areas, teaching, research and community services as indicated in figure 3. here follow examples of measures and norms to determine proficiency on the attainment of goals on these three areas: • teaching 1. staff teaching performance: student feedback = external examiners = average assessment of students for a particular course average assessment of students all courses in the department number of appointments of μ + d examiners for individual staff members 2. staff quality: qualification ratio •• total number of appointments of m + d number of staff members with doctorates total number of academic staff in department 3. student performance: completion ratio = students: completion of course in four years students completion of course in more than four years die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 s.r. hugo & w.m.j. hugo average pass mark: students in 1st, 2nd, 3rd and 4th year in ^ _ speech ther/aud average pass ma average pass mark: students in the faculty man-hours) and a outputs of the department of speech therapy and audiology (hugo & van rooyen, 1990, ρ 298). most academic departments have only three major resources available: manpower, finances and facilities (see figure 3). • research 1. output: publications = n u m h e r of accredited papers this year average number of accredited papers in last 3 years productivity =_ total number of research publications this year man-years allocated to research this year student activity j^umi>er of active doctoral students /year number of registered doctoral students /year 2. quality: international ratio = composition of research = number of internationally published articles this year total number of articles published by the department number of research papers: basic /applied / dissertation / monographs /accredited/ proceedings / reports 3. impact: citations = total number of papers / publications published number of citations in research publications in past 3 years number of citations in this field: science citations index • community service 1. involvement: number of staff on boards of representation associations/institutes this year membership fees paid by department 2. continued education: number of students registered certificate programmes = year number of students the previous year 4.2.3.2.2 efficiency evaluation of efficiency is aimed at determining how well the department is utilising the scarce resources at its disposal. the measures applied for efficiency essentially depict the ratio between the inputs (for example, • manpower 1. workload: input ratio = distribution =. number of postgraduate students qualified / year number of professors in department / year number of assignments marked by individual lecturer average number of assignments for all lecturers 2. development: number of conferences attended conference attendance = fry all staff total number of academic staff study leave = n u m b e r of staff on sabbatical /year total number of academic staff 3. growth of academic staff: growth rate = growth rate of academic staff / 5 years growth rate of students registered/ 5 years number of staff who acquired qualification ratio ^doctoral degrees in the past 2 years average number of staff in the department ι • finances | i 1. budget objectives: i total budget allocation for conference conferences = attendance | number of conferences attended by staff/year i i total budgeted cost for photocopying = photocopying / year actual budgeted cost for photocopying / year 2. availability of finances: increase in student subsidy units ratio of financing = in past 3 years increase iryactual departmental budget in past 3 years total budget allocated to total budget = department/year total academic staff complement /year the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) departmental evaluation: speech therapy and audiology 33 • facilities 1. utilisation: computertime library = actual number of hours utilised on research mainframe computer budgeted number of hours on research mainframe available /year total number of loan requests for department /year total number of academic staff in department 2. availability: computerisation = number of pc's available in department total number of academic staff in department area of office space available to accommodation = department number of students registered in 4.2.4 information gathering and quantifying deparmental outcomes (results) in this step basic data are collected and analysed using the measures and norms identified. in step 3 it may be necessary to review some of the measures and norms during this exercise because the process of gathering information sometimes reveals relevant aspects of performance that were ignored in the planning phase, or alternatively, the data required for specific measures and norms may not be available. 4.2.5 evaluating actual departmental performance this step involves the actual evaluation, judgment or assessment of the evaluation data generated in step 4 in order to draw a conclusion about the worth or value of departmental performance, j four major approaches can be used in judging or assessing actual departmental! performance (adapted from worthen & sanders, 1987 ρ 302 and hugo & van rooyen, 1990, ρ 302): j • comparison referenced assessment performance is judged against other academic departments • criterion referenced assessment performance is compared to absolute standards • objective-referenced assessment actual performance compared to planned performance or specified performance objectives • historically referenced assessment actual performance is compared to own previous performance or performance trends based on historical data. 4.6 reporting the evaluation report aims at providing evaluation information (based on quantitative and qualitative elements of the evaluation procedure) to relevant authorities. as in the case of an academic department, these authorities may be departmental management, faculty management or political authorities. it is essential that the report should be drafted with reference to the purpose and objectives of the evaluation plan. based on evaluation information, it should also indicate areas or activities where improvement is recommended and should reflect possible future developments. 5 conclusion the evaluation model proposed above is a conceptual model designed to be used for and by the a department of speech therapy and audiology. it is essential to realise that in the implementation of the model, contextual influences unique to each department will have to be considered in structuring appropriate measures and norms. the principles on which the model is based are not new, and its only merit may perhaps be found in a logical structure for the measurement of departmental performance. measuring outcomes of educational practices is a modern phenomenon. valuing their worth is as old as philosophy itself. house, 1986 ρ 1 references departement van nasionale opvoeding. (1987). akademiese standaarde by universiteit in die rsa. nasop 02-129 (87/ 10). pretoria. emmet, t.a. & bennett, j.b. (1988). five indices of program vitality. the department advisor. 1(5), 6-8. feldman, a.s. (1981). the challenge of autonomy. asha, 941945. goedegebuure, l.c.j., maasen, p.a.m. & westerheijden, d.f. (ed.). (1990). peer review and performance indicators. utrech: lemma. goodlad, s. (1985). educating for the professions. surrey: srhe & nfer-nelson. house, e.r. (ed.). (1986). new directions in educational evaluation. london: falmer press. hugo, w.m.j. & van rooyen, d.c. (1990). purchasing and materials management. 2nd.ed. pretoria: j.l. van schaik. kells, h.r. (1989). self-study processes. 3rd edition. new york: macmillan. kogan, m. (1986). education accountability. london: hutchingson. kogan, m. (ed.). (1989). evaluating higher education. london: jessica kingsley. lee, l.j. & sampson, j.f. (1990). apractical approach to program evaluation. evaluation and program planning. 13, 157-164. middleton, j., terry, j. & bloch, d. (1989). building educational evaluation capacity in developing countries. washington: world bank. mortimore, p. & stone, c. (1990). measuring educational quality. british journal of educational studies, xxxix(l), 69-82. nichols, j.o. (1989). institutional effectiveness and outcomes assessment implementation on campus: a practitioners handbook. new york: agathon press. nisbet, j. (1988). in the scottish council for research in education, (ed.). the evaluation of educational programmes: methods, uses and benefits. amsterdam: swets & zeitlinger. nowlen, p.m. (1988). a new approach to continuing education for business and the professions. new york: macmillan publishing company. seldin, p. (1988). evaluating and developing administrative performance: a practical guide for academic leaders. london: jossey-bass. wirgin, j.f. & braskamp, l.a. (1987). evaluating administrative services and programs. san francisco: jossey-bass. wolf, r.m. (1990). evaluation in education: foundations of competency assessment and programme review. 3rd.ed. new york: preager. worthen, b.r. & sanders, j.r. (1987). educational evaluation: alternative approaches and guidelines. new york: longman. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 zodiac 901, another star in the madsen constellation zodiac 901 offers the best of both worlds 1. fast automatic performance of tympanometry and acoustic reflex testing for routine screening. 2. complete impedance testing capabilities, preprogrammable automatic test sequences, coupled with the refinement of sensitive manual testing. children's headset clinical tests screening tests zodiac 901 lets you get the most out of middle-ear analysis = pty ltd the hearing health care specialists swf house sovereign street bedfordview 2008 south africa p.o. box 630 . bedfordview 2008 south africa tel: (011)622-1743 fax: (011) 622-1306 reg. no. 77/01577/07 amtronix breaking the sound barrier the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 199 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 41 stemprobleme by enkele groepe professionele stemgebruikers: implikasies vir voorkoming anita van der merwe, marita van tonder, estelle pretorius en helga crous departement kommunikasiepatologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming primere voorkoming van kommunikasieafwykings word toenemend 'n belangrike beroepsfunksie van die spraak-taalterapeut. funksionele stemhiperfunksie wat dikwels voorkom by professionele stemgebruikers kan voorkom word. in hierdie artikel word gesamentlik verslag gedoen oor drie onafhanklike ondersoeke na stemprobleme by drie groepe professionele stemgebruikers. daar het 183 persone in onderwysprofessies, 50predikante en 20 sangers aan die studie deelgeneem en die inligting is versamel met vraelyste. die resultate toon aan dat stemprobleme en simptome van stemprobleme by aldrie groepe voorkom, dat hulle weinig kennis oor die onderwerp het, hulselfblootstel aan risikofaktore en nie hul stemme konserveer nie. riglyne vir voorkomingsonderrig word verskaf. abstract the primary prevention of communication problems is a growing part of the professional responsibility of the speechlanguage therapist. functional voice hyperfunction, which often occurs in professional users of voice, can be prevented. in this article three separate studies on voice problems of people in three different professions are reported. the subjects included 183 educators, 50 ministers and 20 singers. the data were obtained using questionnaires. the results indicate that voice problems and symptoms of voice problems occur in all three groups, that these speakers have little knowledge of the subject, that they expose themselves to high risk factors, and that they do not conserve their voices. guidelines for instruction in prevention are provided. sleutelwoorde: stemprobleme, professionele stemgebruikers, stemhiperfunksie, voorkoming. histories gesproke is die primere beroepsfunksie van die spraak-taalterapeut die habilitasie of rehabilitasie van kommunikasie-afwykings. 'njbelangrike klemverskuiwing het egter die afgelope paar jaar voorgekom in die beroepbeskrywing. die kons'epte van primere, sekondere en tersiere voorkoming is bekendgestel met gevolglike uitbouing van die beroep. tersiere voorkoming verwys na wat tradisioneel beskou is as habilitasie of rehabilitasie van 'n reeds bestaande kommunikasieafwyking. sekondere voorkoming verwys na die vroee identifikasie van 'n probleem met die doel om die effek daarvan te verhoed of te beperk. primere voorkoming daarenteen verwys na die voorkoming van 'n probleem wat nog nie ontstaan het nie (gerber, 1990; marge, 1991). die behandeling van 'n reeds bestaande probleem is arbeidintensief en duur en in 'n gemeenskap soos in suid-afrika met beperkte fondse en mannekrag spreek dit vanself dat voorkoming meer voordelig sal wees. nie alle kommunikasie-afwykings kan primer voorkom word nie, maar die stemprobleme van professionele stemgebruikers, wat 'n groot persentasie van stemprobleme uitmaak (brodnitz, 1988), kart wel voorkom word. hierdie persone wat bestaan uit politici, dosente, onderwysers, predikante en enige-ander persone wat hul stemme professioneel gebruik, beoefen dikwels foutiewe stemgewoontes en kom soms op' 'n punt waar hulle nie verder die beroep kan beoefen nie of lang periodes siekverlof moet neem as gevolg van die • stemprobleem. verlies van hierdie hoogopgeleide persone is 'n onnodige ekonomiese verlies vir 'n land. vir die persoon self hou die verswakking of verlies van die stem nie alleen ernstige implikasies vir die beoefening van sy beroep in nie, maar ook sosiale en emosioneel-persoonlike implikasies. funksionele stemhiperfunksie kom nie alleen by professionele stemgebruikers voor nie. enige volwassene of kind kan sy stem misbruik en dan 'n stemprobleem ontwikkel. ook in hierdie gevalle sal voorkoming meer effektief wees as genesing (child & johnson, 1991). primere voorkoming vereis begrip van die onderliggende etiologie van 'n toestand (gerber, 1990). funksionele stemhiperfunksie, maar ook bydraende organiese en emosionele faktore gee aanleiding tot die stemprobleme van professionele stemgebruikers (van der merwe, 1982). "tesame hiermee is daar ook die eis van stemgebruik vir baie ure per dag. die foutiewe stemgewoontes wat kan voorkom by hierdie persone word deur boone (boone, 1983; die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 4, 199 • re pr od uc ed b y sa bi ne t g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 anita van der merwe, marita van tonder, estelle pretorius en helga crous boone & mcfarlane, 1988) beskryf as funksionele stemhiperfunksie en verdeel in hiperfunksie in fonasie, respirasie en resonansie. die foutiewe gebruik word ook soms verdeel in stemwangebruik en stemmisbruik (child & johnson, 1991). eersgenoemde verwys na die foute in fonasie, respirasie en resonansie terwyl laasgenoemde verwys na gewoontes soos gil, hoes en keelskoonmaak. in essensie is beide egter vorms van stemhiperfunksie waar oormatige spierspanning gebruik word. psigogene faktore kan bydra tot hiperfunksionele gedrag (aronson, 1985), maar spanning en angs kan ook as gevolg van die stemuitputting ontstaan en aanleiding gee tot 'n bose kringloop (brodnitz, 1988). omgewingsfaktore soos stof en gasse lei ook dikwels tot irritasie van die stemstrukture (child & johnson, 1991) terwyl lawaai veroorsaak dat die persoon onnodig hard moet praat. professionele stemgebruikers stel hulself dikwels bloot aan hierdie en ander risikofaktore wat voorkombaar is. voorbeelde van professionele stemgebruikers wat hulle stemme vir lang periodes per keer moet gebruik, wat dikwels in ongunstige omgewingsomstandighede moet optree en wat spanningsvolle situasies moet hanteer, is predikante, persone in onderwysprofessies en sangers. hierdie groepe ontvang wel tot 'n mate onderrig in stemgebruik, maar dit is ook persone uit hierdie groepe wat dikwels na spraaktaalterapeute verwys word as gevolg van stemprobleme wat hulle ondervind. dit wil dus voorkom asof hierdie groepe teikenpopulasies is vir voorkomende optrede deur die spraak-taalterapeut. ten einde te bepaal of stemprobleme by hierdie groepe professionele stemgebruikers in suid-afrika voorkom en of hulle kennis het omtrent die voorkoming van stemprobleme, is drie onafhanklike studies uitgevoer by persone in onderwysprofessies, by predikante en by sangers. in hierdie artikel word gesamentlik verslag gedoen oor die inligting wat bekom is en riglyne word gegee vir voorkomende optrede deur die spraak-taalterapeut. metode doelstellings die doel van die drie studies was om te bepaal: of stemprobleme en simptome wat dui op stemprobleme voorkom by enige van die professionele stemgebruikers wat by die studie betrek is en of hierdie persone kennis dra omtrent stemprobleme, hulself blootstel aan risikofaktore en of hul enige voorkomende stappe neem. navorsingsmetode as metode van data-insameling is selfbeoordelingsvraelyste gebruik. drie verskillende vraelyste is opgestel ten einde relevante data vir elke groep te bekom. proefpersone die enigste kriterium vir seleksie van proefpersone is dat hul steeds hul beroep aktief beoefen. ouderdom verwante invloed op die stem word sodoende sover as moontlik beperk. afrikaanssprekende persone is betrek β aangesien die vraelyste slegs in afrikaans opgestel is. die proefpersone is almal werksaam in die stedelike gebiede van gauteng. drie verskillende groepe stemgebruikers is in die drie studies gebruik: studie 1 (crous, 1991): persone uit verskillende onderwysprofessies, naamlik 61 hoerskoolonderwysers, 57 laerskoolonderwysers, 42 tegnikondosente «.en 23 universiteitdosente het deelgeneem aan die studie. die responspersentasies was onderskeidelik: 96%, 100%, 93% en 46%. studie 2 (van tonder, 1995): predikante uit die drie sogenaamde afrikaanse susterkerke is geselekteer aangesien die preekstyl baie ooreenkom. vyftig persone het deelgeneem aan die studie. die responspersentasie was ongeveer 90%. studie 3 (pretorius, 1994): sangers wat hoofsaaklik in afrikaans sing, populere liedere sing en nie noodwendig formele sarigonderrig gehad het nie, is genader om aan die studie deel te neem. die sangstyl van die sangers is sodoende gekontroleer ten einde heterogeniteit te beperk. twintig sangers tussen die ouderdornme van 20 en 55 jaar is betrek. materiaal drie vraelyste met relevante vrae vir elke spesifieke groep is opgestel. die vrae is in verskillende afdelings verdeel ten einde die verlangde inligting te bekom soos aangedui in die doelstellings. die vrae het dus gehandel oor die voorkoms van stemprobleme by die respondente, die blootstelling aan risikofaktore wat uniek is by elke groep, die hoeveelheid stemgebruik, voorkomende optredes en hul kennis omtrent stemgewing is ook indirek nagegaan. by elke vraag is alternatiewe antwoorde verskaf en die respondent moes aandui of die antwoord op hom van toepassing is deur ja of nee te antwoord. byvoorbeeld, die aard van stemprobleme wat reeds ervaar is, is gelys en die respondent moes dan 'n keuse maak tussen die alternatiewe antwoorde. die vraelyste aan die drie groepe is nie identies nie. die omstandighede van elke groep verskil en nie alle vrae is relevant vir elke groep nie. verder is die data bekom uit drie studies wat oor 'n aantal jare uitgevoer is en insig in die belang van sekere aspekte (soos blootstelling aan risikofaktore) is eers mettertyd besef op grond van die vorige studies. die vraelyste is in samewerking met statistici ontwikkel ten einde effektiewe kodering te verseker tydens die analise van die data en ook om te verseker dat 'die vraelyste voldoen aan die algemene vereistes gestel aan vraelyste. die vraelyste bestaan uit kort ondubbelsinnige vrae waarop die persoon slegs ja of nee moes antwoord. die voltooing moes nie langer as 15 minute in beslag neem nie en dit is ook anoniem ingevul. die drie vraelyste gaan nie volledig weergegee word nie aangesien dit te omvattend sal wees en aangesien die vrae blyk uit die tabelle waarop die resultate aangeteken is. die volledige vraelyste is wel beskikbaar in die genoemde drie studies. die vrae en beoordeling van die vrae berus op erkende feite aangaande stem en stemafwykings soos in die literatuur aangegee. analise van die data die frekwensie van voorkoms van elke alternatiewe antwoord in die drie verskillende vraelyste, is bepaal. die the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) stemprobleme by enkele groepe professionele stemgebruikers: implikasies vir voorkoming data van die verskillende afdelings van die vraelyste en die vrae wat daaronder resorteer word in afsonderlike frekwensietabelle weergegee. die data van die drie erskillende studies is onafhanklik verwerk en word a f s o n d e r l i k aangebied. hierdie frekwensietabelle word subjektief beskryf en bespreek. beskrywing van resultate die resultate van elke onderskeie studie word afsonderlik beskryf: studie 1: vraelyste aan persone in onderwysprofessies die resultate van die ondersoek na die stemgebruik van persone in verskillende onderwysprofessies word weergegee in tabelle 1 tot 3. 43 in tabel 1 is die resultate van onderwysers en dosente geskei aangesien die aantal ure van onderrig oor die algemeen verskil en dus 'n invloed sal he op die voorkoms van probleme. in tabelle 2 en 3 was hierdie onderskeid nie nodig nie. die syfers in tabel 1 dui aan dat groot getalle van die |persone in onderwysprofessies wel reeds stemprobleme ondervind het. heesheid sonder rede en dus waarskynlik as gevolg van stemmisbruik, het by 23% van onderwysers en 23% van dosente voorgekom. ernstiger probleme soos verlies van stem het by tussen 51% en 7% van respondente gepresenteer. die duur van hierdie probleme was oor die algemeen nie lank nie, maar daar was wel 9 persone by wie dit vir weke of maande voortgeduur het. die grootste persentasie persone ondervind nie ten tyde van die ondersoek probleme nie, maar daar is tog baie persone (7%, 8%, 19% en 6%) wat wel probleme ondervind. simptome wat kan dui op probleme kom in hoe persentasies voor by die twee groepe. tabel 1: stemprobleme en simptome van stemprobleme by die persone in onderwysprofessies vraag moontlike aantal aantal vraag antwoord onderwysers dosente (n=118) (n=65) probleme heesheid met verkoue 88 (75%) 45 (69%) ondervind heesheid sonder rede 27 (23%) 15 (23%) verlies van stem 23 (19%) 15 (23%) geringe stemverlies 51 (43%) 33 (51%) ernstige stemverlies 7 (6%) 7 (11%) duur van kortstondig 59 (50%) 33 (51%) probleem dae 51 (43%) 23 (35%) weke 4 (3%) 3 (5%) maande 0 (0%) 2 (3%) tans 'n probleem ja 8 (7%) 5 (8%) nee 83 (70%) 54 (83%) ; soms 22 (19%) 4 ( 6 % ) kenmerke van asemrig 8 (7%) 2 ( 3 % ) stem gereeld hees 28 (24%) 20 (31%) ofvoortdurend stemverlies 4 (3%) 3 ( 5 % ) hoes 10 (8%) , 5 ( 8 % ) pyn 11 (9%) 8 (12%) i keelskoonmaak 43 (36%) 21 (32%) 1 1 te hard 4 (3%) 6 (9%) te sag 15 (13%) 6 (9%) word gou moeg 20 (17%) 14 (22%) monotoon 1 (1%) 1 (2%) luisteraarja 2 (2%) 5 (8%) klagtes te sag 0 (0%) 3 ( 5 % ) te vinnige spraak 0 (0%) 2 ( 3 % ) stem het ja 14 (12%) 5 (8%) verander harder 9 (8%) 2 (3%) sedert aanvang meer hees 4 (3%) 0 ( 0 % ) van beroep verswak 4 (3%) 2 ( 3 % ) pyn as praat 5 (4%) 1 (2%) krapperig 3 (3%) 3 (5%) gouer moeg 2 (2%) 3 (5%) toonhoogte verander 4 (3%) 3 ( 5 % ) 1 stemverlies 0 (0%) 1 (2%) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 anita van der merwe, marita van tonder, estelle pretorius en helga crou gereelde keelskoonmaak wat gewoonlik 'n teken is van stemongemak (boone & mcfarlane, 1988), kom by 36% en 32% van die groepe voor. daar is by 12% en 8% van die persone 'n bewussyn dat hulle stemme verander het sedert die aanvang van hul beroepe. brodnitz (1988) noem dat 'n spreker gewoonlik die laaste persoon is wat daarvanbewus tabel 2: kennis van stemprobleme by die persone in onderwysprofessies word dat sy stem verander en in die lig hiervan is die genoemde persentasies wel veelseggend. die gegeweris in tabel 2 wat handel oor die kennis van stemprobleme toon aan dat die meeste van die moontlike veroorsakende faktore genoem word, maar dit is oor die algemeen slegs klein getalle van die respondente wat daarvan bewus is. die persentasies van 19% en 12% wat daarvan bewus is dat oormatige gebruik en 'n harde stem probleme kan veroorsaak, is besonder laag en dui op swak kennis van die oorsake van stemprobleme. die feit dat slegs 31% voorheen ingelig is aangaande goeie stemgebruik, bevestig hierdie afleiding. soveel as 81% is bewus daarvan dat stemprobleme wel kan ontstaan, maar vraag moontlike antwoord aantal (n= =183) redes vir die oormatige gebruik 35 (19%) ontstaan van te hard praat 22 (12%) stemprobleme soos foutiewe stemaangegee deur gebruik 9 (5%) respondente rook 8 (4%) (oop vraag) spanning van stembande 3 (2%) praat sonder luidspreker 3 (2%) praat as verkoue'het 14 (7%) skree 9 (5%) mediese redes 7 (4%) langdurige gebruik 13 (7%) allergiee/sinusitis 2 ( 1%) omgewingsfaktore 3 (2%) emosioneel 2 ( 1%) sportafrigting 8 (4%) werkomstandighede 2 (1%) blameer vir foutiewe ontstaan van 'n stemgewoontes · 54 (30%) stemprobleem beroepseise 100 (55%) omgewing 23 (13%) rook/besoedeling 52 (28%) mediese redes 24 (13%) spanning 37 (20%) voorheen ingelig goeie stemgebruik 57 (31%) omtrent die risikofaktore vir volgende ontstaan van 'n stemprobleem 14 (7%) hoe werksituasie aangepas kan word 12 (7%) hoe om 'n stemprobleem te hanteer 15 (8%) instansies waar hulp verkry kan word 6 (3%) was voorheen nooit daaraan bewus van gedink nie 30 (16%) moontlikheid van was bewus stemprobleme daarvan ervaar angs oor moontlike ontstaan 148 (81%) van 'n probleem 9 (5%) die vraelys het ja 108 (59%) bewussyn geskep nee 71 (39%) van stemprobleme tabel 3: optrede en houding teenoor stemprobleme by die persone in onderwysprofessies vraag moontlike aantal antwoord (n= 183) neem stappe om ja 115 (63%) stem op te pas nee 28 (15%) tipe stappe stemrus tydens geneem volgens siekte 2 ( 1%) respondente waak teen (oop vraag) ooreising 3 (2%) nie te hard praat nie 15 (8%) laer "stemtoon" 19 (10%) praat minder 0 (0%) beperk allergiee 7 (4%) voorkom siekte 2 ( 1%) elimineer irritasie 7 (4%) rook nie 1 ( 1%) kry mediese hulp 1 (1%) reaksie op 'n skuldig voel 7 (4%) moontlike dit aanvaar 6 (3%) stemprobleem wegsteek 2 (1%) ignoreer 4 (2%) werk bedank 4 (2%) staak rook 12 ( 7%) stilbly 3 (2%) hulp soek \ 122 (67%) stemeienskappe verander 20 (11%) ontsteld wees 56 (31%) optrede indien 'n na huisdokter stemprobleem gaan 123 (67%) sou ontstaan na spraakterapeut gaan 38 (21%) na onk-arts gaan 42 (23%) verlof neem 5 (3%) stemgebruik beperk 23 (13%) werk volhou / ongeag probleem 27 (15%) bewus dat ja 26 (14%) spraakterapeut nee 138 (75%) met stemprobleme werk the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) tmprobleme by enkele groepe professionele stemgebruikers: implikasies vir voorkoming 45 . w i l voorkom asof kennis van en insig in die probleem k is die vraelys het by 59% 'n bewussyn geskep van mprobleme en was dus waardevol in hierdie opsig. die b e w u s s y n van 'n probleem moet egter aangevul word met ^ uittabel 3 blyk dit dat'slegs 63% van die respondente . stappe neem om hul stemme te konserveer. die tipe f p e wat aangegee is in 'n oop vraag dui ook op beperkte snsig in veroorsakende faktore. relevante stappe word wel 1 noem, deur klein persentasies van die persone, maar daar is'belangriker voorkomende maatreels waarvan hierdie persone bewus behoort te wees. die meeste van die persone sal wel hulp soek as hul probleme ondervind, m a a r 'n onaanvaarbare persentasie (15%) sal hul werk volhou. slegs 'n baie klein persentasie (14%) was bewus daarvan dat die spraakterapeut van hulp kan wees by die bantering van stemprobleme. studie 2: vraelyste aan predikante die resultate van die ondersoek na stemprobleme by 'n groep predikante word saamgevat in tabelle 4 tot 7. uit tabel 4 blyk dit dat 54% van die respondente wel reeds stemprobleme ervaar het. in die meeste gevalle (46%) was die probleme kortstondig, maar daar is 4 persone wat langdurige probleme het. die meeste van die predikante het probleme ondervind as gevolg van laringitis, maar daar is 20 (11 + 9) wat heesheid ervaar het sonder laringitis of verkoue. die oorsaak van laasgenoemde is waarskynlik stemmisbruik. ander aanduidings van stemmisbruik kom ook voor. stemuitputting en pyn in die keelarea kom by 56% voor en 38% se dat hulle gereeld moet keelskoonmaak. die volgende twee vrae oor die ervaring van sekere simptome tydens en na afloop van 'n optrede dui op 'n baie hoe voorkoms van stemmisbruik by hierdie groep. geringe stemuitputting, forseer van stem, heesheid en ander simptome kom by die meeste persone voor. slegs 7 (14%) het aangedui dat hul wel pyn ervaar (wat ook kan dui op uitputting), maar geen hoorbare stemverandering het nie. die inligting in tabel 5 toon aan dat die predikante ook blootgestel word aan ander risikofaktore as bloot baie stemgebruik. agtergrondslawaai (by 72%), rook en gasse (by 34%) en swak akoestiek met onvoldoende luidsprekerstelsels (by 74% en 70%) kom voor waar hulle optree. verder stel hulle hulself ook bloot deur lang stukke voor te lees (50%) en doelbewus hul stemeienskappe te tabel 4: stemprobleme en simptome van stemprobleme by predikante vraag moontlike antwoord aantal n=50 stemprobleme ervaar het al stemprobleme ervaar kortstondige probleme langdurige probleme beide kortstondige en langdurige probleme 27 (54%) 23 (46%) 2 ( 4 % ) 2 ( 4 % ) simptome ervaar 1 i ! 1 kortstondige stemheesheid tydens verkoue of laringitis kortstondige stemheesheid sonder verkoue of laringitis stemheesheid vir 'n paar ure tydens verkoue of laringitis stemheesheid vir 'n paar ure sonder verkoue of laringitis stemheesheid vir 'n dag of langer tydens verkoue of laringitis stemheesheid vir 'n dag of langer sonder verkoue of laringitis stemverlies tydens verkoue of laringitis stemverlies sonder verkoue of laringitis stemuitputting pyn in keelarea gewoonte om gereeld keel skoon te maak ander stemprobleme organies dui waarskynlik op stemmisbruik 43 (86%) 15 (30%) 36 (72%) 11 (22%) 35 (70%) 9 (18%) 6 (12%) 2 ( 4 % ) 28 (56%) 28 (56%) 19 (38%) 5 (10%) 3 ( 6 % ) 2 ( 4 % ) negatiewe kommentaar negatiewe kommentaar aangaande stemkwaliteit 2 ( 4 % ) ervaar tydens optrede geringe stemuitputting voel asof ek my stem forseer heesheid tree in behoefte om gereeld keel skoon te maak pyn in keel, maar geen verandering in stemkwaliteit 19 (38%) 10 (20%) 11 (22%) 14 (28%) 7 (14%) ervaar na optrede geringe stemuitputting heesheid behoefte om gereeld keel skoon te maak pyn in keel, maar geen verandering in stemkwaliteit 27 (54%) 12 (24%) 15 (30%) 7 (14%) stem verander sedert aanvang van beroep 1 geringe veranderinge groot veranderinge 9 (18%) 1 ( 2 % ) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 anita van der merwe, marita van tonder, estelle pretorius en helga cro verander, soos die verhoging van luidheid (52%). wat kennis omtrent stemprobleme betref (sien tabel 6), blyk dit dat die predikante beperkte kennis het. dit blyk onder meer uit 'n beskrywing van wat 'n stemprobleem is en wat subjektief deur die ondersoeker beoordeel is op grond van erkende literatuur. in antwoord op die vraag of daar enige gevolge is indien stem misbruik word, dui 88% aan dat daar geen gevolge is nie en 12% is onseker. die helfte van die groep besef wel dat fisiese veranderinge kan voorkom, maar hulle verstaan dus nie wat die implikasies sal wees nie. hul kennis oor die meganisme van luidheidverhoging is ook beperk. slegs 40% gee die korrekte antwoord, naamlik dat dit nie moontlik is om luidheid te verhoog deur verhoogde nekspierspanning nie (boone & mcfarlane, 1988). net 30 (60%) van die respondente besef dat emosionele faktore 'n tabel 5 die blootstelling aan risikofaktore by predikante vraag moontlike antwoord aantal (n= =50) omstandighede agtergrondslawaai waar optree teenwoordig rook en gasse 36 (72%) teenwoordig 17 (34%) swak akoestiek 37 . (74%) luidsprekerstelsel afwesig of onvoldoende 35 (70%) doen die volgende lees lang stukke hardop voor 25 (50%) gebruik doelbewus 'n ander stem 3 (6%) gebruik 'n preekstem wat hoer of laer in toonhoogte ' i s as my normale praatstem 2 (4%) gebruik 'n preekstem wat harder in luidheid is as my normale praatstem 26 (52%) rookgewoontes rook 10 of minder sigarette per dag 2 (4%) rook meer as 10 sigarette per dag 5 (10%) rook glad nie 43 (86%) stemgebruik in as ek op reis is in 'n voertuig voertuig gesels ek die heeltyd met die passasiers 6 (12%) as ek op reis is in 'n voertuig gesels ek soms met die passasiers 43 (8%) as ek op reis is in 'n voertuig gesels ek glad nie 1 (2%) invloed het op stem (aronson, 1985). riglyne oor g 0 e i e stemgebruik is wel ontvang (76%), maar slegs 12 (24%) het dit van 'n spraakterapeut ontvang. die moontlikheid bestaan dat dit nie werklik 'n spraakterapeut was nje aangesien die kennis van die publiek oor die algemeen nie baie goed is in hierdie verband nie. in tabel 7 word die optrede en houding teenoor stemprobleme saamgevat. die grootste persentasie (98%) besef wel dat stemprobleme kan voorkom by predikante en soveel as 34% is altyd bekommerd oor die moontlikheid en 62% is nou en dan bekommerd. hierdie hoe persentasies dui aan dat hulle onseker is oor die wyse waarop 'n probleem voorkom kan word. die stappe wat hulle neem is by klein persentasies van die groep wel relevant tabel 6: kennis van stemprobleme by predikante vraag moontlike antwoord aantal vraag (n=50) eie beskrywing geen kennis 6 (12%) van stemprobleem beperkte kennis 28 (56%) goeie kennis 8 (16%) geen antwoord 8 (16%) kennis van tydelike stemprogevolge van stembleme kom voor: 41 (82%) misbruik ja 1 (2%) nee 8 (16%) onseker fisiese veranderinge aan stembande kom voor: ja 25 (50%) nee 24 (48%) onseker 1 (2%) daar is geen gevolge nie: ja 0 (0%) nee 44 (88%) onseker 6 (12%) verhoogde luidi ja, dit is moontlik heid verkry deur slegs in sommige 14 (28%) spanning in gevalle 7 (14%) nekspiere glad nie j 20 (40%) weet nie 1 9 (18%) het emosionele ja, beslis 30 (60%) faktore 'n nee, glad nie 11 (22%) invloed? onseker 9 (18%) riglyne oor geen riglyne 12 (24%) stemgebruik beperkte riglyne 31 (62%) tydens opleiding volledige riglyne 7 (14%) riglyne ontvang spraakonderwyser 11 (22%) van spraakterapeut 12 (24%) dosent in spraakleer en drama 8 (16%) dosent by teologie departement 5 (10%) sangonderwyser 2 (4%) onseker 2 (4%) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mprobleme by enkele groepe professionele stemgebruikers: implikasies vir voorkoming 47 tabel rj. optrede en houding teenoor stemprobleme "j vraag moontlike antwoord aantal (n=50) pas stemrus toe wend pogings aan om doelbewuste stemrus toe te pas 6 (12%) stemprobleme by predikante ja, dit is. moontlik onseker nee, dit kom selde voor 49 (98%) 1 (2%) 0 (0%) bekommerd oor stem ek is altyd bekommerd daaroor ek is nou en dan bekommerd daaroor ek is glad nie bekommerd daaroor nie 17 (34%) 31 (62%) 2 (4%) stappe tot konservering neem stappe om stem op te pas 31 (62%) tipe stappe geneem medikasie gereelde stemrus beperkte luidheid beperkte hoeveelheid stemgebruik 20 (40%) 10 (20%) 19 (38%) 17 (34%) reaksie op moontlike stemprobleem ophou werk vir 'n tydperk en gaan hulp soek ophou werk vir 'n kort tydperk aanhou werk, maar gaan hulp soek aanhou werk, sonder, om hulp te soek j 13 (26%) 3 (6%) 43 (86%) 0 (0%) vorige hulp ontvang huisdokter homeppaat oorn;eusen keelarts kollegas spraakterapeut elokusie-onderwyser geen hulp gesoek nie 36 (72%) 3 (6%) 10 (20%) 3 (6%) 3 (6%) 2 (4%) 12 (24%) wie in toekoms raadpleeg huisdokter homeopaat oor-, neusen keelarts kollegas spraakterapeut elokusie-onderwyser 40 (80%) 2 (4%) 31 (62%) 2 (4%) 23 (46%) 3 (6%) spraakterapeut kan help ja nee onseker 46 (92%) 1 (2%) 3 (6%) (byvoorbeeld 10 pas gereelde stemrus toe), maar 'n groot groep (20) is van mening dat medikasie van waarde sal wees. in reaksie op 'n moontlike stemprobleem dui al die predikante aan dat hulle hulp sal gaan soek en selfs sal ophou werk vir 'n periode. die grootste persentasie (72% en 80%) het in die verlede en sal weer in die toekoms na hul huisdokter gaan vir hulp. dit wil ook voorkom asof groter persentasies (20% teenoor 62% en 6% teenoor 46%) die hulp van 'n oor-, neus-, en keelarts en spraakterapeut sal inroep vir moontlike toekomstige probleme. groter bewussyn is moontlik geskep met die vraelys of die persone het uit ervaring geleer dat hierdie twee professies in samewerking die beste bystand kan bied. studie 3: vraelyste aan sangers die resultate van die ondersoek na stemprobleme by 'n groep sangers word weergegee in tabelle 8 tot 12. in tabel 8 word die voorkoms en simptome van stemprobleme by hierdie groep saamgevat. kortstondige probleme na 'n vertoning kom by 5 (25%) van die groep tabel 8: stemprobleme en simptome van stemprobleme by sangers vraag voorkoms van stemprobleme agteruitgang van stem probleme tydens optredes moontlike antwoord geen stemprobleem ervaar kortstondig, na vertoning probleme na. verkoue/laringitis 1/meer episodes vir weke/maande geen verandering sedert aanvang geringe veranderinge in stem groot veranderinge in stem geringe uitputting • herstel gou forseer stem heesheid tree in keelskoonmaak ligte pyn, geen veranderinge asemrigheid geeneen van bogenoemde probleme na afloop van optrede geringe uitputting • herstel gou heesheid ook in spraakstem ligte pyn, geen veranderinge keelskoonmaak geeneen van die bogenoemde aantal (n=20) (25%) (25%) 10 (50%) 4 (20%) 13 (65%) 5 (25%) 2 (10%) 10 2 13 5 2 2 (50%) (10%) (65%) (25%) (10%) (10%) (20%) 14 (70%) 12 (60%) 2 (10%) 3 (15%) 2 (10%) die suid -afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) anita van der merwe, marita van tonder, estelle pretorius en helga crou 48 voor en soveel as 4 (20%) het een of meer episodes van probleme ervaar wat vir weke of maande voortgeduur het. daar was ook agteruitgang in stemkwaliteit by 7 (35%) van die groep. slegs 4 (20%) sangers het geen stemprobleme tydens optredes nie terwyl slegs 2 geen stemprobleme het na afloop van optredes nie. dit is dus duidelik dat stemuitputting weens stemmisbruik by die meeste van die sangers voorkom. die inligting in tabel 9 toon aan dat die sangers hulself wel blootstel aan risikofaktore. slegs 3 (15%) berei voor vir optredes. die luidheid van begeleiding word net deur 7 (35%) sangers beperk en 14 van die groep gebruik nie gereeld monitor-luidsprekers wat terugvoer van die stem verbeter nie. die gevolg sal dus wees dat hulle stemluidheid uitermate moet verhoog ten einde voldoende terugvoer te kry. daar is selfs 2 sangers wat nie altyd klankversterking gebruik nie en 8 (40%) gesels baie in vervoermiddels waar lawaai ook stemterugvoer beperk. addisionele omgewingsfaktore soos geraas en rook word slegs deur 11 (55%) van die sangers beperk. die enigste positiewe saak is dat geen een van die sangers meer as 10 sigarette per dag rook nie. die meeste (18) rook gladnie. wat kennis omtrent stem en stemprobleme betref, is daar persone wat weinig kennis het. vyf (25%) het geen sangopleiding ontvang nie en daar is ook 5 wat van mening is dat die stemomvang met meer as een register verhoog kan word. dit is nie die geval nie aangesien die bou van die stembande die register en toonhoogte van die stem bepaal (colton & casper, 1990). 'n verdere aanduiding van swak kennis van stemgewing blyk uit die feit dat 5 (20%) nie besef dat verhoogde nekspierspanning nie nodig is vir verhoogde luidheid nie (colton & casper, 1990; stemple,1984). soveel as 13 het geen of weinig riglyne oor stemkonservering ontvang tydens opleiding. die meeste sangers is wel bewus van waiter gewoontes potensieel skadelik is vir die stem, maar dit is verrassend dat almal nie skadelike gewoontes kan identifiseer nie. byvoorbeeld, slegs 7 besef dat dit skadelik is om sonder versterking te sing en slegs 12 (60%) besef dat keelskoonmaak potensieel skadelik is (boone & mcfarlane, 1988). tabel 10: kennis van stemprobleme by sangers tabel 9: die blootstelling aan risikofaktore by sangers vraag moontlike antwoord aantal (n=20) voorbereiding oefen toonlere en vir optrede doen ander voorbereidende oefeninge 3 (15%) oefen slegs paar toonlere 9 (45%) geen voorbereiding 8 (40%) luidheid van beperk luidheid 7 (35%) begeleiding beperk luidheid in 'n mate 10 (50%) beperk luidheid glad nie 2 (10%) monitorgebruik altyd luidspreker monitor-luidspreker 5 (25%) gebruik soms monitor-luidspreker 11 (55%) gebruik nie monitorluidsprieker 3 (15%) klankgebruik altyd versterking versterking 18 (90%) buite met en binne sonder versterking 1 (5%) geen klankversterking 1 (5%) toestande tydens geen geraas en rook 11 (55%) optrede tydens optredes min geraas en rook 8 (40%) gedurige geraas en rook 1 ( 5%) rook rook gladnie 18 (90%) rook minder as 10 sigarette per dag 2 (10%) rook meer as 10 sigarette per dag · 0 (0%) stemgebruik in gesels die minimum 12 (60%) vervoermiddel gesels voortdurend 8 (40%) vraag moontlike antwoord aantal (n=20) sangopleiding green sangopleiding individuele sangopleiding kooropleiding 5 (25%) 11 (55%) 4 (20%) opleiding in voorkoming volledige riglyne ontvang beperkte riglyne ontvang geen riglyne oor stemkonservering 7 (35%) 11 (55%) 2 (10%) meganisme vir luidheid-verhoging nekspanning nie nodig vir luidheid nekspanning somtyds nodig nekspanning nodig vir luidheid 14 (70%) ί (5%) 4 (20%) potensieel skadelik vir stem keelskoonmaak skadelik vir stem hard sing sonder versterking sing buite natuurlike omvang geen opwarming van die stem 12 (60%) 7 (35%) 13 (65%) 16 (80%) stemomvangvergroting stemomvang binne perke vergroot verhoog met 1 register deur oefen verhoog met meer as 1 register 15 (75%) 1 (5%) 5 (25%) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) me by enkele groepe professionele stemgebruikers: implikasies vir voorkoming 49 tabel 11· stemkonservering deur sangers vraag moontlike antwoord aantal (n=20) stappe in stemkonservering pas stemrus toe gebruik medikasie neem geen stappe om stem op te pas nie 16 (80%) 9 (45%) 3 (15%) stemrus tydens pouses totale stemrus tydens pouses gesels wel, maar min gee nie aandag aan stemrus nie 1 (5%) 9 (45%) 6 (30%) duur van optredes minder as 1 uur tussen 1 en 2 ure langer as 2 ure 6 (30%) 13 (65%) 1 (5%) frekwensie van optredes tree 1 of 2 kere per week op tree 3 of 4 kere per week op tree meer as 4 keer per week op 12 (60%) 7 (35%) 1 (5%) opeenvolging van optredes tree nooit op opeenvolgende dae op nie slegs sekere weke op opeenvolgende dae dikwels op opeenvolgende dae 2 (10%) 13 (65%) 4 (20%) duur van sangoefening geen formele sangoefening ongeveer 20 minute per dag ongereelde oefening langer'as 1 uur per dag | 8 (40%) i (5%) ii (55%) 0 (0%) rusperiodes tydens optredes altyd rjusperiodes soms rjusperiodes geen rusperiodes nie 3 (15%) 9 (45%) 8 (40%) ander beroep geen addisionele beroep | beoefen addisionele beroep 8 (40%) 12 (60%) gebruik in ander beroep gebruik stem nooit te veel gebruik stem met tye gebruik stem baie in ander beroep 1 (5%) 3 (15%) 8 (40%) hoeveelheid spraakstem gebruik beperk stemgebruik gee nie aandag aan hoeveelheid nie 9 (45%) 11 (55%) luidheid van spraakstem beperk altyd stemluidheid beperk luidheid as stem uitgeput is gee geen aandag aan luidheid nie 1 (5%) 10 (50%) 10 (50%) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, op grond van die voorafgaande inligting is dit te wagte dat baie min van die sangers wel hul stemme konserveer (sien tabel 11). nege (45%) van die proefpersone reken dat medikasie kan bydra tot stemkonservering. slegs 1 pas totale stemrus toe tydens pouses en 8 (40%) tree drie of meer kere per week op. soveel as 17 (85%) tree soms op agtereenvolgende dae op wat weinig geleentheid vir die stembande bied om te herstel na uitputting. slegs, 3 (15%) neem altyd rusperiodes tydens optredes. sangoefening word deur al die sangers afgeskeep en 12 beoefen addisionele beroepe waar 8 hulle stemme baie gebruik in die uitoefening van die beroep. ook die spraakstem word nie gekonserveer nie. elf (55%) gee geen aandag aan die hoeveelheid gebruik nie en 10 gee geen aandag aan die luidheid van die spraakstem nie. die optrede en houding teenoor stemprobleme by sangers word in tabel 12 weergegee. veertien (70%) sal wel ophou sing indien hulle probleme ondervind en 19 sal tabel 12: optrede en houding teenoor stemprobleme by sangers vraag moontlike antwoord aantal (n=20) reaksie op stemprobleem verwelkom nuwe stemkwaliteit ophou sing en gaan hulp soek ophou sing vir 'n kort tydperk aanhou sing en gaan hulp soek 1 (5%) 9 (45%) 5 (25%) 5 (25%) wie raadpleeg huisdokter homeopaat oorneusen keelarts kollegas of agente sangonderwyser spraakterapeut 8 (40%) 11 (55%) 1 (5%) 5 (25%) 5 (25%) bewussyn van probleem stemprobleme kan by sangers voorkom onseker nie bewus van stemprobleme nie 20 (100%) bekommernis oor stem altyd bekommerd oor stemprobleme nou en dan bekommerd glad nie bekommerd nie 6 (30%) 8 (40%) 6 (30%) kennis van gevolge gevolge: ligte pyn en stemuitputting fisiese veranderinge in die stembande onseker oor gevolge van stemprobleme 4 (20%) 12 (60%) 6 (30%) invloed van spraakstem spraakstemprobleem bei'nvloed sangstem bei'nvloed moontlik die sangstem 13 (65%) 7 (35%) vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 anita van der merwe, marita van tonder, estelle pretorius en helga crous mediese hulp verkry. al die sangers is bewus daarvan dat stemprobleme by sangers kan voorkom, maar 14 (70%) bekommer hulle nie baie oor die moontlikheid nie. ses (30%) is onseker oor die gevolge van stemprobleme en slegs 12 (60%) besef dat fisiese veranderinge kan intree in die stembande indien die stem misbruik word.,slegs 13 (65%) besef dat 'n probleem met die spraakstem beslis ook die sangstem sal bei'nvloed (colton & casper, 1990). bespreking van resultate dieselfde tendense is waargeneem by die drie groepe. stemprobleme' en simptome van stemprobleme kom in wisselende grade voor by al drie die groepe professionele stemgebruikers wat betrek is by die studie. kennis oor stemgewing en stemprobleme is beperk en die persone stel hulself bloot aan allerlei risikofaktore sonder om aandag te gee aan stemkonservering. hulle is wel bewus van die moontlikheid van 'n stemprobleem en sommige is selfs angstig dat hulle so 'n probleem sal ontwikkel, maar beskik nie oor die nodige kennis om stemgebruik te monitor nie. hoewel hierdie studies slegs onderneem is by enkele professionele groepe, is dit wel moontlik dat dieselfde resultate verkry sal word by ander professionele en taalgroepe. die enigste ander studie (sover bekend) oor stemmisbruik by professionele sprekers in suid-afrika is die van reynolds (1994) wat die bewussyn van stemmisbruik by finalejaar studentonderwysers van twee universiteite in kwa-zulu natal ondersoek het. meer as die helfte van die 209 studente wat deelgeneem het aan die studie het aangedui dat hul bewus is van stemuitputting en stemprobleme en soveel as, 19 het tydens hul eerste blootstelling aan die praktyk hul stemme verloor. die resultate van hierdie studie bevestig dus die bevindinge van die huidige studie, naamlik dat stemprobleme wel voorkom by professionele stemgebruikers. stemprobleme kan voorkom word deur riglyne vir optimale stemgebruik in te sluit by die kurrikula vir opleiding van persone wat hul stemme professioneel gaan gebruik, deur publieke opleiding deur middel van die openbare media en deur inligting aan professionele persone deur die spraak-taalterapeut. dit is nie alleen professionele sprekers wat by voorkomingsaksies baat sal vind nie, maar ook die bree publiek wat ander groepe stemgebruikers ook insluit (heidel & torgerson, 1993; nilson & schneiderman, 1983; aaron & madison, 1991). die essensie van voorkoming van stemmisbruik en 'n gevolglike stemprobleem-, is volgens johnson (1991) die self-monitering van gedrag. die persoon moet dus opgelei word om self sy stem te konserveer. ten einde 'n persoon daartoe in staat te stel is daar egter sekere sake wat aangespreek moet word in 'n opleidingsprogram en sekere vereistes waaraan dit moet voldoen: die relevansie van die onderwerp vir die gehoor moet gemotiveer word. die persoon moet dus die noodsaaklikheid van stemkonservering insien en verstaan dat voorkoming 'n eie verantwoordelikheid is. navorsingsresultate omtrent stemprobleme by soortgelyke groepe kan kortliks genoem word asook enkele gevallestudies. basiese feite oor stemgewing moet kernagtig, maar wetenskaplik oorgedra word ten einde insig en begrip te vestig vir normale en abnormale stem. die verskillende oorsake van stemprobleme, naamlik primer-organiese, psigogene en funksionele oorsake met en sonder sekondere organiese gevolge kan net genoem word. die noodsaaklikheid van 'n ondersoek deur 'n oor-, neusen keelarts indien enige stemprobleem voorkom, moet te alle tye beklemtoon word. die persoon moet die aard van stemhiperfunksie verstaan. die vorms van stemmisbruik in fonasie, respirasie en resonansie moet verduidelik word sodat die persoon verstaan wat swak stemgewoontes behels en dus instaat sal wees tot identifisering daarvan in eie stemgebruik. die persoon moet die oorsake van stemhiperfunksie verstaan ten einde risikofaktore te kan identifiseer en uitskakel. die rol van oorsake soos foutiewe stemgewoontes en oormatige gebruik, omgewingsen fisiese faktore soos lawaai, gasse en allergiee en emosionele faktore moet verduidelik word. die simptome of "waarskuwingstekens" van stemmisbruik moet genoem word: stemuitputting met ongemak of selfs pyn wanneer stem geproduseer word; heesheid en selfs stemverlies vir 'n periode nadat stem gebruik is; die behoefte om gedurig keel skoon te maak wanneer die stem gebruik word; 'n beter stem soggens en na naweke en swakker stem later in die dag en later in die week. die risikofaktore vir die spesifieke groep kan uitgelig word. in die geval van dosente of onderwysers is daar onder andere swak luidsprekerstelsels, swak akoestiek, praat in die teenwoordigheid van studentelawaai en die uitdrogende effek van bordkryt. die stappe vir voorkoming wat geneem kan word behoort ook prakties uitgelig te word teen die agtergrond van die inligting wat verskaf is. die stappe kan die volgende behels: beheer, verander of vermy omgewingsfaktore wat aanleiding gee tot stemmisbruik; gee aandag aan emosionele faktore wat spanning veroorsaak en soek hulp indien die probleem ernstig word; verminder die hoeveelheid stemgebruik deur onder andere rusperiodes; beheer fisiese faktore soos rook, sooibrand en die oormatige inname van stowwe soos kafeien en koladranke (aronson, 1985; child & johnson, 1991); skakel stemmisbruik en wangebruik uit. dra belangrike algemene reels vir optrede'indien 'n probleem reeds voorkom, oor: die spesifieke faktore wat aanleiding gee tot 'n stemprobleem, moet by elke persoon individueel gei'dentifiseer word en dan sistematies uitgeskakel word; daar moet opgetree word voordat die probleem sover gevorder het dat 'n stemband-operasie nodig is; soek hulp by 'n oor-, neusen keelarts en dan ook by 'n spraak-taalpatoloog indien 'n stemprobleem wel ontwikkel. hierdie is algemene riglyne vir voorkomingsonderrig vir stemprobleme. enkele meer spesifieke programme vir spesifieke groepe is in die literatuur beskikbaar (aaron & madison, 1991; kaufman & johnson, 1991; nilson & schneiderman, 1983). gevolgtrekking die resultate van hierdie ondersoek toon aan dat die groepe wat ondersoek is, almal hoerisikogroepe is vir die ontwikkeling van stemprobleme. binne die konteks van 'n land met beperkte hulpbronne, is dit dus belangrik dat die plaaslike spraak-taalterapeut voorkomingsonderrig vir the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s t e m p r o b l e m e b y e n k e l e g r o e p e professionele s t e m g e b r u i k e r s : i m p l i k a s i e s v i r v o o r k o m i n g 51 stemprobleme sal inisieer as deel van die beroepsfunksies w a t vervul moet word. verwysings aaron, v.l. & madison, c.l. (1991). a vocal hygiene program for high-school cheerleaders. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 22, 287-290. a r o n s o n , a.e. (1985). clinical voice disorders. new york: thieme. boone, d.r· (1983). the voice and voice therapy (3rd ed.). new jersey:-prentice-hall. boone, d.r. & mcfarlane, s.c. (1988). the voice and voice therapy (4th ed.). new jersey: prentice-hall. brodnitz, f.s. (1988). keep your voice healthy (2nd ed.). boston: college-hill. child, d.r. & j o h n s o n , t.s. (1991). preventable and nonpreventable causes of voice disorders. seminars in speech and language: prevention of voice disorders, 12, 1-13. colton, r.h. & casper, j.k. (1990). understanding voice disorders. baltimore: williams & wilkins. crous, h. (1991). 'n ondersoek na stemprobleme by die professionele stemgebruiker in sekere onderwysprofessies. ongepubliseerde b.log-verhandeling, universiteit van pretoria. gerber, s.e. (1990). prevention: the etiology of communicative disorders in children. new jersey: prentice hall. heidel, s.e. & torgerson, j.k. (1993). vocal problems among aerobics instructors and aerobic participants. journal of communication disorders, 26, 179-191. johnson, t.s. (1991). principles and practices of prevention as applied to voice disorders. seminars in speech and language: prevention of voice disorders, 12, 14-22. kaufman, t.j. & johnson, t.s. (1991). an exemplary preventative voice program for educators. seminars in speech and language: prevention of voice disorders, 12, 40-48. marge, m. (1991). introduction to the prevention and epidemiology of voice disorders. seminars in speech and language: prevention of voice disorders, 12, 49-72. nilson, h. & schneiderman, c.r. (1983). classroom program for the prevention of vocal abuse and hoarseness in elementary school children. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 14, 121-127. pretorius, e. (1994). stemkonservering by 'n groep professionele sangers. ongepubliseerde b.log-verhandeling, universiteit van pretoria. reynolds, m.a. (1994). an investigation into self-awareness of vocal abuse with special reference to final year student-teachers. ongepubliseerde m.a.-verhandeling, universiteit van natal. stemple, j.c. (1984). clinical voice pathology: theory and management. columbus: charles e. merrill. van der merwe, a. (1982). a hierarchical analysis of voice pathology: a guide to diagnosis and treatment. the south african journal of communication disorders, 29, 16-22. van tonder, m. (1995). stemgebruik by 'n groep predikante. ongepubliseerde b.kommunikasiepatologie-verhandeling, universiteit van pretoria. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a neuropsychological approach to the study of gesture and pantomime in aphasia jocelyn kadish, b.a. (sp. & h. th.) (witwatersrand) dept. logopaedics. university of cape town. summary t h e impairment of gesture and pantomime in aphasia was examined from a neuropsychological perspective. the boston diagnostic test of aphasia, luria's neuropsychological investigation, pickett's tests for gesture and pantomime and the performance scale of the wechsler adult intelligence scale were administered to six aphasic subjects with varying etiology and severity. results indicated that severity of aphasia was positively related to severity of gestural disturbance; gestural ability was associated with verbal and non-linguistic aspects of ability, within receptive and expressive levels respectively; performance on gestural tasks was superior to that on verbal tasks irrespective of severity of aphasia; damage to luria's second and third functional brain units were positively related to deficits in receptive and expressive gesture respectively; no relationship was found between seventy of general intellectual impairment and gestural deficit. it was concluded that the gestural impairment may best be understood as a breakdown in complex sequential manual motor activity. theoretical and therapeutic implications were discussed. opsomminc die versteuring van gebare is vanuit 'n neuropsigologiese oogpunt ondersoek in ses proefpersone met afasie. verskillende oorsake en grade van aantasting het voorgekom navorsing is deur middel van die "boston diagnostic test of aphasia", luria se "neuropsychological investigation", pickett se "test for gesture and pantomime" en die "wechsler adult intelligence scale" gedoen. resultate dui op 'n positiewe verband tussen graad van afasie en die graad van gebareversteuring. die vermoe om gebare te gebruik hou verband met die verbale en nie-iinguistiese vermoens op beide die reseptiewe en ekspressiewe vlakke. desnieteenstaande die graad van afasie, is in die ondersoek gevind dat die vaardigheid om gebare te gebruik beter was as die vaardigheid op η verbale vlak. beskadiging van luria se tweede en derde funksionele breineenhede hou positief verband met η gebrek in reseptiewe en ekspressiewe gebare onderskeidelik. geen verband is gevind tussen die graad van algemene intellektuele versteuring en gebareversteuring nie. dus kan die versteuring in die gebruik van gebare beskou word as η belemmering in die komplekse opeenvolgende motoriese aktiwiteit van die hande. implikasies vir die teorie en terapie is bespreek. the role of gestures in communication has been demonstrated'by several a u t h o r i t i e s 2 ' 4 ' 7 ' " · 2 0 · 4 8 and has been crystallised by vetter5'1· when he stated that "gestures are language". however, speech pathologists have, for a long time, recognised speech as the only "true form" of language in the aphasic population. in the desire to elicit verbal communication, the clinician frequently overlooks the aphasic patient's need to communicate in some fashion.19·48 the present study was concerned with an aspect of non-verbal communication, namely, gesture and piantomime, which will be used the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 at gesture and pantomime in aphasia svnonomously to refer to a condition in which a person uses a series of s e q u e n t i a l manual movements, in the absence of speech, in an apparent attempt to communicate.43 a number of experimental studies on non-clinical populations17'222 3 ' 2 5 ' 2 6 ' 2 8 have suggested that a strong relationship exists between speech and manual motor activity. for example, kimura22' 23>24' 5<26 has attributed he relationship to the neural overlapping of cerebral areas which control speech and gesture and has further suggested that both gestural and speech activity are sub-classes of a more general mechanism for the control of sequentially organised motor activity. within this theoret.ca framework, kimura2 5 has derived and validated the proposition that aphasia and gestural disturbance are related. a considerable body of literature dealing with a p h a s i c 1 ' 1 0 ' 1 2 ' a n d deaf aphasic p o p u l a t i o n s 5 ' 9 ' 2 5 ' 4 5 ' 4 9 has further supported the idea of a relationship between speech and manual communicative behaviour for example, goodglass and kaplan1 5 have suggested that gestural deficit s e essentially ipraxic disturbances. however liepman's concept of ideamotor apraxia suggests the existence of an ideat.ona component in addition to an essentially apraxic component. there are also a^number of fairly extreme ideational theories which have viewed gesture and pantomime as part of a general asymbolia25 or in terms of a generalised non-specific intellectual deficit.15 the close relationship between gesture and speech has also been conceptualised within an evolutionary and ontogenetic framework. the work of hewes19 has lent considerable credence to the hypothesis that a manual communicative system developed prior to the vocal communicative system in a phylogenetic sense. from an ontogent.c point ol view mcniell37 has stated that gestures appear during piaget s sensory m o t o r operational stage. mcniell cites as evidence the fact that during the holophrastic stage utterances are typically produced together with an ongoing action. the nature of the present study required that a formal distinction be made between the recognition of gesture (receptive gesture) and performance of gesture (expressive gesture). receptive gesture is regarded as distinct from the afferent control of gestural praxis at a k i n e s t h e t i c level and "receptive gesture" is here regarded as a visuoperceptive phenomenon. in· more exact terms, receptive gesture involves the perceptual recognition of sequentially occunng manual postures within a visuospatial system of coordinants, and the translation of this perception into a symbolic system. within the theoretical system of luria,31 receptive gesture involves the translation of sequential recognition processes into instantaneous recognition and their incorporation into a simultaneously perceived logical scheme to he extent that this holds true, receptive gesture is subserved by the secondary occipital cortex, parietal cortex and temporo-paneto-occ.p.tal functions, the structures representing luna s second functional unit. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings. vol. 25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 104 jocelyn kadish because receptive gesture involves both analytic and synthetic perceptual processes it would appear to depend on both left and right cerebral hemispheres. expressive gesture may be defined as the encoding of information transmitted via the use of sequential motor movements which are superimposed on manual postures and are unaccompanied by speech therefore, expressive gesture may be conceptualised as a phenomenon which is subordinated to the demands of goal directedness and symbolic processes.3' it is therefore essentially dependant on the integrity of luna's third functional unit which is comprised of the frontal and prefrontal motor systems.31 to the extent that gestural praxis is brought into correspondence with symbolic or prepositional constraints, it is also dependant on the integrity of the parietal tertiary corteses. the prepositional and temporal nature of expressive gesture suggests its dependance upon the left hemisphere. this conceptualisation of receptive and expressive gesture within luria's neuropsychological framework forms the basis for the present research and contains several implications for a clinical approach to aphasia. the major aims of the present study were to examine the relationship between gestural deficit and integrity of neuropsychological systems as determined by means of luria's neuropsychological investigation3 and to examine the relationship between gestural deficit and severity of aphasic disturbance. a subsidiary aim was to explore possible relationships between residual intellectual efficiency and gestural competence. method subjects (ss) six aphasic patients of varying etiology and severity were selected on the basis of the following criteria:ss were to 1. have been diagnosed as aphasic by a neurologist and speech therapist this was to be further confirmed by a formal rating on the boston diagnostic test of aphasia.16 2. have suffered their cerebral insults at least six months prior to testing and were thus regarded as neuropsychological^ s t a b l e . 8 ' 4 5 ' 5 2 3. be native speakers of the english language. 4. show no evidence of sensory deficits in the auditory, visual and tactile modality. / 5. come from similar cultural backgrounds and have 'a minimal educational level (std. 8). 6. have passed two preliminary screening tests adapted from pickett4 3 which determine abilities in recognition and naming of objects and tactile-visual matching tasks prerequisites for the gesture tests to be used in the present study. description of subjects see table i for relevant clinical and biographical the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesture and pantomime in aphasia 105 ss age mean age age range sex education level home language premorbid occupation occupation changed due to illness laterality hemiplegia pathology si s2 48,1 46,4 57,83 46 years 4 months to 70 s3 6,3 s4 59,11 f std. f μ university degree ε civil maniengineer curist years 2 months f f std. 8 std.i r ν (recovered) trauma followed by removal of tissue due to infection r r vascular arteriosclerosis in carotid artery. hypercholesterolmic saleslady r r i) loss of sensation ii) oral apraxia months post onset therapy at ambulant recovered r side y 37 sp. & h. clinic y 36 sp. & h. clinic r side s5 70,2 μ matric cerebro vascular accident varied. saleslady collector ν collector r ν (recovered) aphasia cerebral thrombosis followed by absces in left inferior carotid artery. surgery resulted in right hemiparesis r side ν 168 company director l r vascular arteriosclerosis etiology: hypertension j.g. stryd sp. & h. hospital clinic s6 59,6 f matric typist bookkeeper r r cerebrovascular accident r side y 7 bramley old age home ν r side y 99 sp. & h. clinic key: μ = male l = left f = female y = yes ε = english ν = no r = right table l. relevant biographical and clinical information pertaining to the subjects (ss). testing the following tests were administered :1. gesture and pantomime tests, adapted from pickett. this test was selected from currently available t e s t s 1 0 ' 1 2 ' 1 5 ' 4 3 ' 4 4 as it die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 106 jocelyn kadish tests both comprehension and expression of gesture; makes use of homogeneous items between tests; capitalises on all sensory modalities to elicit performance and hence eliminates the contamination of an impaired sensory modality on performance potential; and, allows for verbal as well as non-verbal means of communication in giving the instructions and hence facilitates optimal comprehension about the nature of the task. description of gesture tests: each test was centred around 10 commonly used homogeneous objects, contained in the porch index of communicative ability.43 two test items were modified so as to render them suitable for the south african population. the tests were graded in order of extent of information given about the test. ss were cautioned not to talk during the administration of the tests. gestural test 1 (gl) — expressive: ss were presented with a picture of an object and were required to pantomime the function. gestural test 2 (g2) expressive: ss were given the actual object to use in pantomime. gestural test 3 (g3) — receptive: ss were presented with pictures of persons in postures associated with the function of the test objects and were required to point to the appropriate object. gestural test 4 (g4) — expressive: ss were given auditory instructions to pantomime. gestural test 5 (g5) — receptive: the ε pantomimed the functions of each object, and the ss were required to point to the appropriate object. gestural test 6 (g6) — imitative: the ε pantomimed the functions of an object, and subsequently, the ss were required to imitate the e. 2. luria's neuropsychological investigation.3 this was administered so as to yield a measure of functioning at various neuropsychological levels; to localise the lesions of each subject; and, to gain an understanding of the basic factors upon which gesture depends and the critical structures involved. the following subtests were selected on the basis of their relevance to this study:z 7 (i) motor functions , (ii) acoustico-motor organisation (iii) higher cutaneous and kinaesthetic functions (iv) higher visual functions (v) impressive speech (vi) expressive speech ' (vii) mnestic processes 1 the south a frican journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesture and pantomime in aphasia 107 in addition, two total scores were obtained for visuo-spatial functions and dynamic organisation since it was hypothesized that these functions are involved in receptive and expressive gesture, respectively. the totals of the following subtests were used to yield the two measures. (viii) visuo-spatial functions: kinaesthetic basis of movement optico-spatial organisation of the motor act stereognosis objects and picture recognition spatial orientation intellectual operations in space form recognition size contrast effects immediate reproduction of visual, acoustic, kinaesthetic and verbal traces (ix) dynamic organisation: dynamic organisation of the motor act oral praxis speech regulation of the motor act scoring a set of optimal criteria (optimality range) was drawn up for each of the 44 subtests. a score of 1 was assigned to each optimal criterion which corresponded to the s's performance e.g. for the subtest entitled opticospatial organisation, 4 optimal criteria were drawn up:(i) muscle power and tone (ii) accuracy of movements (iii) symmetry (iv) immediacy of response thus, a s who performed within this range would get a score of 4. the optimality ranges were compiled on the basis of christenson3 and luria.30 3. boston diagnostic test of aphasia.16 to obtain an unequivocal diagnosis of aphasia and an objective severity.rating of the ss' linguistic abilities. 4. the performance scale of the "wechsler adult intelligence scale.52 to yield a measure of performance i.q., the latter defined as a deviation score on the wechsler adult intelligence scale. results and discussion the principle statistical analysis applied to the present data was the spearman rank order correlation coefficient and the associated tests of significance.35 because of the small number of subjects involved in the present study, apparently high correlations may be non-significant and further, the non-significant correlations cannot be interpreted in a totally die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 108 jocelyn kadish unambiguous fashion. hence, only those correlations which were significant were interpreted as reflecting a systematic relationship and the non-significant correlations, no matter how high the r value, were interpreted as being no different from zero. gesture the scores obtained on the gesture tests are represented in table ii. table iii represents the matrix of intercorrelations between the gesture tests. the findings presented in table iii suggest that a gestural impairment encompasses a deficit which incorporates the manipulation of real and pretended objects. the deficit clearly extends from the level of the formulation of a motor sequence to the execution of that sequence, and additionally involves imitation. the significant correlation coefficients obtained between receptive (g3; g5) and expressive (gl) gesture tests suggests the existence of a general underlying process which represents gestural competence, i.e. reception and expression seem to be part of a continuum as opposed to being dichotomous alternatives. receptive expressive gesture gesture st 20 463 52 20 453 53 17 404 54 20 476 55 7 118 56 20 417 ν maximum score 20 480 table ii. scores obtained on receptive and expressive gesture tests for all ss. g1(e) g2(e) g3(r) g4(e) g5(r) g6(l) (e)g1 0,81426» 0,857143* 0,942857» 0,857143» 0,942857* (e)g2 0,58714 0,9* 0,585714 0,671429 (r)g3 0,714286 1» 0,914286* (e)g4 0,714286 0,885714* (r)g5 0,914286* (1)g6 key: ε = expressive gesture ' r = receptive gesture * = significant correlation ( r s 0,829: ν — 6; ρ 0,05). spearman correlation coefficient of at least 0,829 is required for significance (p = 0,05) when ν = 6. 1. — imitation j table iii. spearman rank-order intercorrelation coefficients between the six gesture tests. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesture and pantomime in aphasia a ε s o cx υ χ cx ω ι λ cx υ s ο. « cλ • g o < 2 _ ο g s ' l .a μ 5 c q..7 3 ** [λ ui ' s e 5 ο ο qo 3 00 o" o" 00 o" 00 ο ο β 1) u u1 q. <λ (u u u os ο ω ο cn cn rrt o t o 00 00 cn cn s s ο ο to — vo ο ω υ υ υ ' π ora qu a υ > g cx * cx u •s " >. > 8q < die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 110 jocelyn kadish neuropsychological functions and gesture. although a general gestural competence has been postulated, it is nevertheless true in terms of the model proposed that they are dependant on separate neurological subsystems and therefore the question of whether they are dependant on different neuropsychological functions becomes relevant. spearman rank-order correlations were obtained between gesture (receptive and expressive respectively) and several neuropsychological functions as tested on luria's neuropsychological investigation.3 (see table iv). the findings suggest that receptive gestural ability is closely and differentially related to visuo-spatial functioning and mnestic ability. there is therefore an association between receptive gesture and the various perceptual processes which are dependant upon luria's unit 2 i.e. the posterior secondary and tertiary corteses.30 this finding then suggests that receptive gesture, in common with other perceptual processes, is similarly dependant on unit 2. in terms of the present framework, the absence of a correlation between expressive gesture and dynamic organisation and acousticomotor organisation, respectively, is surprising since gesture is often regarded as a form of praxis.3 0'3 1 however, it is clear that future research is required to elucidate this problem. case studies detailed results obtained from tests administered for each subject are summarised in table v. although detailed case studies formed a large part of the present study, it is not within the scope of this paper to discuss them in depth. for the purposes of this article, only one case will be presented in terms of what is directly relevant to the stated aims. subject 2 (s2) (a) s2 showed symptoms indicative of a lesion of the posterior frontal or fronto-temporal areas of the left hemisphere, manifesting as symptoms typical of "transcortical aphasia"3 3 i.e. difficulty in connected motor speech, breakdown occurring when extent of repetition is broadened to several words, difficulty in the smooth transition between words, and symptoms of pathological inertia.3 furthermore, there was evidence of a breakdown in "kinetic melodies"32 of motor skills, writing difficulties and confusion between articulemes, all of which were suggestive of a lesion of the lower portion of the premotor zone.32 while simple motor functions of the hands seemed unimpaired, the latter lesion seemed to manifest in apparent difficulties in the dynamic organisation of movement functions. s2 performed well on acoustico-motor functions, cutaneous and kinaesthetic functions and mnestic processing tests, (the latter corresponding to an "average" performance iq of 100) which suggests intact functioning of the posterior regions of the brain. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesture and pantomime in aphasia 111 receptive tests gesture language neuropsychological functions visuomnestic acouscutaneous spatial processticoand organiing motor kinestic sation percepperception tion si 20 23 26 27 6 17 s2 20 35 29 27 10 19 s3 17 21 18 4 2 12 s4 20 23 31 20 7 18 s5 7 2 15 4 4 7 s6 20 24 31 14 8 18 maximum 10 19 score 20 42 45 31 10 19 x = 17,3 expressive tests gesture language neuropsychological functions motor dynamic acoustico functions organimotor of hand sation performance si 463 37 16 26 12 s2 453 48 18 29 13 s3 404 3 17 18 3 s4 476 36 17 31 10 s5 118 13 6 15 3 s6 417 45 13 31 13 maximum 13 score 480 58 22 44 13 x = 338,5 tests subjects performance scale of the wechsler adult intelligence scale si s2 s3 s4 s5 s6 110 100 28 96 32 89 boston diagnostic aphasia test severity rating 3 3 0 3 1 2 t a b l e v. scores obtained for gestural, linguistic and neuropsychological tests, on receptive and expressive levels respectively, in addition to performance iq, and severity rating. die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 112 jocelyn kadish (b) the areas implicated above seemed to overlap with those presumed to subserve expressive gesture, while the posterior cerebral areas concerned with receptive gesture appeared intact. hence, it may be predicted that some difficulty on expressive gesture would be evident, associated with no apparent receptive gesture difficulties. (c) as demonstrated in table iv, scores yielded by the gesture tests confirmed the above i.e. a score of 453 out of a maximum score of 480 obtained for expressive gesture, while a score of 20 out of a maximum of 20 obtained for receptive gesture. summary of case studies 1. dynamic localisation of a lesion affords one the opportunity to predict a s's receptive and expressive ability on gestural tasks. (a) lesions of the posterior divisions of the cerebral cortex invariably manifest as an impairment on receptive gesture tasks. this was clearly evident in si, s3 and s5. (b) lesions of the anterior divisions of the brain result in expressive gestural impairments as was the case in all ss. 2. the relation between verbal language and gesture was demonstrated in that all ss manifested concomitant deficits on both receptive and expressive levels. 3. for all subjects, performance on verbal language tests was superior to that on gestural tests. gesture and speech a significant correlation coefficient was obtained for impressive speech and receptive gesture (r s = 0,8714286; ν = 6; ρ 0,05). this has been supported by several authorities all of whom attributed the relationship to an underlying communication breakdown. 1 0 ' 4 3 however, within a neuropsychological framework the results assume another ' dimension. according to luria,30 the areas of the brain subserving impressive speech include posterior-superior and middle zones of the left temporal region, temporo-occipital and temporo-parieto-occipital zones of the left hemisphere. the latter two areas were found, in the present study, to be a part.of the mechanism responsible for the comprehension of gesture. thus speech and gesture, on a receptive level, appear to have a similar neurological basis. κ at an expressive level, the absence of a relationship between gesture and speech is not consistent with the framework proposed here and again must be a subject of future research. an important finding of the present study was that gestural language suffers considerably less than does verbal language, on both receptive and expressive levels, irrespective qf the severity of the aphasia. this contrasts with those of duffy et al1 0 who found inferior performance on pantomime recognition tests irre'spective of whether they were pathological or not. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesture and pantomime in aphasia 113 the sparing of gestural communication relative to the verbal counterpart may be viewed in a number of ways: firstly, in terms of the ontogenetic framework proposed by mcnielf and hacean1 8 aphasia may be viewed as a regression to piaget's sensori-motor stage. this theory would lend support to "ribot's rule" which states that in the dissolution of aphasia the last acquired functions are first to be involved, while the first acquired, offer the greatest resistance to extinction.42 a second interpretation, although somewhat less plausible than the first one, may be derived from the socio-cultural evolutionary framework proposed by hewes19 which hypothesises that gesture occurs earlier in the evolutionary sequence than speech or independently of speech. in this context, aphasia may be viewed as a regression to an earlier phylogenetic stage. finally, one may view this finding in terms of minkowski's principle38 that stressed the "affectional, psychological and emotional" background of language and suggested that language supporting emotional content will recover best. this seems particularly relevant to the language of gesture which has been reported to be an important vehicle for the transmission of emotionally charged information.2 residual intellectual capacity and gestural ability intellectual efficiency can no longer be ignored as a factor which contributes to the capacity to perform gesture and pantomime,15 spearman rank-order correlation coefficients obtained between scores yielded by the performance scale of the wechsler adult intelligence scale53 and receptive and expressive gesture respectively failed to reach the 0,05 level of significance. (r = 0,8; ν = 6; ρ 0,05) and (r s = 0,7714286; ν = 6; ρ 0,05). this suggests that performance on the intelligence test did not predict gestural ability, either receptively or expressively. however, these results must be viewed with caution since it is important to bear in mind the criticisms levelled against the reliability of non-verbal intelligence tests when applied to the brain damaged p o p u l a t i o n . 3 6 ' 4 3 ' 5 0 aphasia and gesture defining a gestural deficit as part of an aphasic communication disorder requires that it be correlated with the severity of aphasia.spearman rank-order correlation coefficients computed between severity of aphasia and receptive and expressive gesture respectively were significant: (r = 0,8571429; ν = 6; ρ 0,05) (r s = 0,8857143; ν = 6; ρ 0,05). this finding suggests that a gestural impairment is a component of the aphasic syndrome and concurs with findings by duffy et al1 0 and pickett.43 however, the latter authors regarded gesture and aphasia to be related on a linguistic or cognitive level, through symbolism or representation. on the basis of criteria enumerated by goodglass and kaplan1 5 and within the present framework, the findings seem to lend credence to the following suggestions:die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 114 jocelyn kadish 1. the notion of a complex sequential motor movement disorder was not supported by results of the present study, and the tentative nature of the present finding does hot allow a definitive interpretation with respect to this relationship. however, this conclusion receives some support from the fact that (a) there was a strong relationship between formulation of gestures and expression of gestures which was not accounted for by the verbal impairment and (b) there was a relationship between manipulation of real objects in addition to pretended ones which, according to goodglass and kaplan1 5 conforms to the definition of a movement disorder. in the light of these findings, the notion of asymbolia seems unlikely but can not be entirely discounted. 2. the close associations found between impressive speech and receptive gesture, from statistical analysis as well as from case study data, suggests that these two functions are dependant on common underlying neurological substrate. however, this interpretation cannot as yet, in terms of the present findings, be extended to expressive gesture and expressive speech. at an expressive level, no association was found between speech and gesture on statistical analysis, even though a positive relationship was found in the case studies. the finding of a strong relationship between verbal language and gesture in general, supports the idea of a common underlying neurological substrate. moreover, the evidence relating to overlapping areas of brain function involved in visuo-spatial functioning, mnestic processes, impressive speech and receptive gesture and the evidence that a lesion of the left cerebral cortex manifested in concomitant expressive verbal language and expressive gestural language impairments, further served to render this hypothesis compelling. finally, the relationship between severity of aphasia and gestural ability, may also serve as evidence for this conclusion. conclusion the neuropsychological approach adopted served to highlight the reliance of receptive and expressive gesture on certain areas of the brain. 1. receptive gesture subserved by the posterior regions of the brain encompassing the secondary occipital cortex, occipitoparietal cortex, parieto-temporal and temporo-parieto-occipital area. hence receptive gesture appears to be linked with luria's second functional unit.31 2. expressive gesture subserved by the anterior regions of the brain, incorporating the basal areas of the cerebral cortex, postcentral regions, premotor divisions and frontal systems. hence this seems to involve luria's third functional unit of the cerebral cortex.31 the conclusion that a gestural impairment is a part of the aphasic syndrome would necessitate a broader view of aphasia than many the south african journal of communication disorders, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesture and pantomime in aphasia 115 authorities present.1 0 in addition, the need for a revised conception of the neurological basis of language was indicated. in conclusion however, the present results must be regarded as tentative and further research employing larger samples is indicated. if the hypotheses suggested here are supported by future research, intervention strategies involving gesture may become a valuable part of the therapy regime. references 1. alajouanine, t. and lhermitte, f. (1963): non-verbal communication in aphasia. in disorders of language. de reuch, a. and o'conner, m. (eds.), boston: little, brown. 2. bates, j.a.v. (1975): the communicative hand. in the body as a medium of expression: essays in social studies. benthall, j. and polhemus, t. (eds.), penguin books ltd. london. 3. christenson, a. (1974): luria's neuropsychological investigation. (text and manual) munksgraad. 4. cicourel, a.v. (1975): gestural-sign language and the study of nonverbal communication. in the body as a medium of expression: essays in social studies. benthall, j. and polhemus, t. (eds.), penguin books ltd. london. 5. critchley, m. (1938): aphasia in a partial deafmute. brain, 61, 163169. 6. critchley, m. (1939): the language of gesture. arnold, london. 7. crystal, d. (1975). paralanguage. in the body as a medium of expression: essays in social studies. benthall, j. and polhemus, t. (eds.), penguin books ltd. london. 8. culton, g.l. (1969): spontaneous recovery from aphasia. j. speech hear. res., 12, 825-32. 9. douglass, e. and richardson, j.c. (1959): 'aphasia' in a congenital deaf-mute. brain, 82, 68-80. 10. duffy, r.j., duffy, j.r. and pearson, l.k. (1975): pantomime recognition in aphasics. j. speech hear. res., 18, 105-114. 11. egolf, b.d. and chester, s.l. (1973): non-verbal communication and the disorders of speech and language. asha 15, 511-518. 12. gainotti, g. and lemmo, m.a. (1976): comprehension of symbolic gestures in aphasia. brain and language 3, 451-460. 13. gainotti, g. and tiacci, c. (1971): the relationship between disorders of visual perception and unilateral spatial neglect. neuropsychologia, 9, 451-459. 14. gardner, r.a. and gardner, b.t. (1969): teaching sign language to a chimpanzee. science, 165, 664-672. 15. goodglass, h. and kaplan, e. (1963): disturbance of gesture and pantomime in aphasia. brain, 86, 703-720. 16. goodglass, h. and kaplan, e. (1972): the assessment of aphasia and related disorders. lea and febiger, philadelphia. 17. goodglass, h. and quadfasel, f.a. (1954): language laterality in left-handed aphasics. brain, 77, 521-548'. die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 116 jocelyn kadish 18. hacean, h., de ajouriaguerra, j. and angelergues, r. (1963): apraxia and its various aspects. in problems of dynamic neurology: studies of the higher functions of the human nervous system. halpern, l. (ed.). department of nervous diseases, rothschild hadassah university hospital and the hebrew university hadassah medical school, jerusalem. israel, pp 217-230. 19. hewes, g.w. (1973): primate communication and the gestural origin of language. current anthropology, 14, 5-24. 20. hinde, r.a. (1975): t h e comparative study of non-verbal communication. in the body as a medium of expression: essays in social studies. benthall, j. and polhemus, t. (eds.), penguin books ltd. london. 21. kellog, w.n. (1968): communication and language in the homeraised chimpanzee. science, 162, 423-427. 22. kimura, d. (1973 a): manual activity during speaking — i. righthanders. neuropsychologia 11, 45-50. 23. kimura, d. (1973 b): manual activity during speaking — ii. lefthanders. neuropsychologia 11, 51-55. 24." kimura, d. (1973 c): the assymetry of the human brain. scientific american, 228, 70-78. 25. kimura, d. (1976): the neural basis of language qua gesture. in studies in neurolinguistics. whitaker, h.a. and whitaker, h.a., (eds.). vol ii. academic press, new york. , 26. kimura, d. and archibald, y. (1974): motor functions of the left hemispheres. brain, 97, 337-350. 27. kimura, d. battison, r. and lubert, b. (1976): impairment of nonlinguistic hand movements in deaf aphasics. brain and language, 3, 566-571. 28. kimura, d. and vanderwolf, c.h. (1970): the relation between hand preference and the performance of individual finger movements by left and right hands. brain, 93, 769-774. 29. kortlandt, a. (1973): discussion of paper by hewes, g.w. primate communication and the gestural origin of language. current anthropology. 14, 13-14. 30. luria, a.r. (1970): traumatic aphasia — its syndromes, psychology and treatment. mouton press. the hague. 31. luria, a.r. (1973): the working brain. allen lane, the penguin press books. 32. luria, a.r. (1974): language and brain: towards the basic problems of neurolinguistics. brain and language, 1, 1-14. / 33. luria, a.r. and hutton, j.t. (1977): a modern assessment of the basic forms of aphasia. brain and language, 4, 129-151. 34. mateer, c. and kimura, d. (1977): impairment of nonverbial oral movements in aphasia. brain and language, 4, 262-276. 35. mccall, r.b. (1970): fundamental statistics for psychology. harcourt, brace and world, international edition. 36. mcfie, j. and piercy, m. (1952): intellectual impairment with localised cerebral lesions. brain, 75, 292-311. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gesture and pantomime in aphasia 117 37. mcneill, d. (1976): semiotic extension. in information processing and cognition. solso, r.l. (ed.) chap. 11. the loyola symposium, usa. 38. minkowski, m. (1963): on aphasia in polyglots. in problems of dynamic neurology: studies on the higher functions of the human nervous system. halpern, l. (ed.). department of nervous diseases rothschild hadassah university hospital and the hebrew university hadassah medical school, jerusalem, israel, pp 119-161. 39. moscovitch, m. (1976): on the representation of language in the right hemisphere of right-handed people. brain and language, 3, 47-71. 40. olson, d.r. (1970): cognitive development: the child's acquisition of diagonality. academic press, london. 41. piaget, j. (1962): play, dreams and imitation in childhood. norton, new york. 42. pick, a. (1973): aphasia. translated and edited by brown, j.w., charles c. thomas publisher, usa. 43. pickett, l.w. (1974): an assessment of gestural and pantomimic deficit in aphasic patients. acta symbolica, 5(3), 69-86. 44. porch, b.e. (1967): porch index of communicative ability (volumes i and ii). consulting psychologists press. palo alto, california. 45. sands, e., sarno, m.t. and shankweiler, d. (1967): long-term assessment of language function in aphasia due to stroke. arch. phys, med, rehab., 50, 202-206. 46. sarno, j.e., swisher, l.p. and sarno, m.t. (1969): aphasia in a congenitally deaf man. cortex, 5, 398-414. 47. sarno, m.t., silverman, m.g. and sands, e.s. (1970): speech therapy and language recovery in severe aphasia. j. speech hear. res., 13, 607-23. 48. schlanger, p.h. and schlanger, b.b. (1970): adapting role-playing activities with aphasic patients. j. speech hear. dis., 35, 229-235. 49. tureen, l.l., smolik, e.a. and tritt, j.h. (1951): aphasia in a deaf mute. neurology, 1, 237-244. 50. vega, a. jr. and parsons, o.a. (1963): relationship between sensory-motor deficits and w.a.i.s. verbal and performance scores in unilateral brain damage. cortex, 5, 238-240. 51. vetter, j.h. (1969): language behaviour and communication. f.e. peacock publishers inc., illinois. ch. 14. 52. vignolo, l.a. (1967): evolution of aphasia and language rehabilitation; a retrospective exploratory study. cortex, 1, 344-67. 53. wechsler, d. (1955): the wechsler adult intelligence scale. the psychological corporation, new york. 54. wiener, m„ devoer, s., rubinow, s. and geller, j. (1972): nonverbal behaviour and non-verbal communication. psychological review, 79(3), 185-214. 55. wepman, j.m. (1976): language without thought or thought without language. asha 18(3), 131-136. die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) selected acoustic characteristics of emerging esophageal speech: case study glenn binder, b.a. (sp. & η. th.) (witwatersrand) dept. speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary the development of esophageal speech was examined in a laryngectomee subject to observe the emergence of selected acoustic characteristics, and their relation to listener intelligibility ratings. over a two-and-a-half month period, the data from five recording sessions was used for spectrographic and perceptual (listener) analysis. there was evidence to suggest a fairly reliable correlation between emerging acoustic characteristics and increasing perceptual ratings. acoustic factors coincident with increased intelligibility ratings appeared related to two dimensions: firstly, the increasing pseudoglottic control over esophageal air release; secondly the presence of a mechanism of pharyngeal compression. increased pseudoglottic control manifested in a reduction of tracheoesophageal turbulence, and a more efficient burping mode of vibration with clearer formant structure. spectrographic evidence of a fundamental frequency did not emerge. these dimensions appeared to have potential diagnostic and therapeutic value, rendering an analysis of the patient's developing vocal performance more explicit for both clinician and patient. opsomming die ontwikkeling van esofagale spraak is ondersoek in 'n gelaringektomeerde geval om die voorkoms van geselekteerde akoestiese eienskappe waar te neem, en hulle verhouding tot luisteraar-verstaanbaarheids-beoordelings. gedurende 'n twee-en-'n-half-maand periode, is data van vyf opname sessies gebruik vir spektrografiese en luisteraar-analise. daar is bewyse vir die daarstelling van 'n redelik betroubare korrelasie tussen die akoestiese eienskappe wat te voorskyn kom en die verhoogde perseptuele beoordelings. akoestiese faktore wat saam val met verhoogde verstaanbaarheids beoordelings blyk verbind te wees ten opsigte van twee aspekte: eerstens, die verhoogde pseudoglottiese beheer oor esofagale lugvrylating; tweedens die teenwoordigheid van 'n meganisme van faringale samepersing. verhoogde pseudoglottiese beheer het 'n afname van trageo-esofagale turbulensie tot gevolg, en 'n meer doeltreffende wind-opbreek metode van vibrasie met 'n duidelike formantstruktuur. spektrografiese bewyse van 'n grondtoon het nie te voorskyn gekom nie. dit blyk dat hierdie aspekte 'n potensiele diagnostiese en terapeutiese waarde het, met die moontlikheid van 'n duideliker analise van die pasient se ontwikkelende vokale prestasie vir die terapeut en pasient. esophageal speech is one of two alternative methods of functional postlaryngectomy rehabilitation. investigators over the past few decades have realised the importance of testing its efficacy both subjectively and objectively — one of the more recent objective methods being the use of spectrographic a n a l y s i s . 2 ' 5 ' 6 ' 7 ' 1 0 ' " ' 13>j17>28>29 the purpose of this article is a) to discuss how spectrographic analysis may be used as a diagnostic and therapeutic tool, and also b) to examine the relationship between subjective and objective data obtained. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) emerging esophageal speech 25 it would be interesting to follow the development of speech from a mechanism that at first is halting, and seemingly acquires a great deal of effort to produce faltering sounds, to the effortless smooth production of sustained speech,4 despite brooks hunt and va's4 valid comment, more than a decade has passed with a dearth of information available on the developmental aspects of esophageal speech acquisition. the importance of measuring a laryngectomized patient's progress in treatment has likewise been stressed by very few writers.3'3 1 however, in the general field of vocal pathology, more recent impetus has been provided by rontal, rontal and rolnick20 who have advocated the use of serially-made spectrograms. these would provide the referring ear nose and throat specialist, clinician and patient with a visual display reflecting the nature of the vocal rehabilitation process. the value of assessing rontal et al's20 finding specifically in the progression of esophageal speech development, was strongly indicated. the paucity of literature with regard to the acoustic and perceptual developmental aspects of esophageal speech acquisition is apparent. the establishment of a description of emerging acoustic characteristics correlated with subjective listener ratings, would be of diagnostic, prognostic, and therapeutic value. it would provide the speech therapist with objective, quantitative criteria against which each laryngectomee's esophageal speech development could be assessed. furthermore, the understanding of the relationship between the emerging dimensions would facilitate a more explicit approach to therapy for both clinician and client. the term 'esophageal speech' refers to speech production resulting from an air supply from the vicarious air chamber located within the esophageal lumen. the cricopharyngeus has been localized as the pseudoglottic site. t h e outflowing air is interrupted by crude pseudoglottic vibrations which alternatively open and close the esophageal lumen. the kay sonograph has been used to provide spectrograms which permit an objective, reliable analysis of the phonetic — acoustic elements of esophageal speech. the sonograph provides: a) narrow-band displays in which harmonic structure is highlighted, and b) broad-band displays which highlight formant structure (represented as dark, thick vertical bands). formants are resonances of the supraglottal vocal tract, 'upon which the distinctive quality of vowels and resonant consonants depend.' 9 (see figure 5, 5-9, and 11-14, a and c, along the horizontal and vertical axes respectively). kerr and lanham9 have used broad-band spectrograms to analyze esophageal speech, and have described the following phenomena which have been corroborated by other investigators:10' " ' l 3 ' 2 9 parameter i: vibratory cycles resulting from the manner of esophageal sphincter control over air release. a. rapping: a term used by kerr and lanham9 to refer to the slow, irregular, rap and tap-like pulses received by the vocal tract functioning die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 glenn binder as a resonating system. (see figure 6, 1,8,9,10,11,17,18,19 along the horizontal axis). b. esophageal burping: the raps smooth out into a succession of air puffs more rapid and regular, and lower in amplitude . . . still lacking in harmonic structure.9 these more rapid cricopharyngeal vibrations facilitate the emergence of a clearer formant structure (see figure 3, 611, a and b, along the horizontal and vertical axes respectively). c. friction noise: this random, aperiodic ill-defined turbulence emanates from the more or less open esophageal sphincter. friction noise has been found to play a major role in producing quality changes 9 ' 1 0 (see figure 6, 20-22 along the horizontal axis). d. possible absence of fundamental frequency: fundamental frequency is the physical correlate of pitch perception,1 0 and is a measure of the frequency of components of periodic waves. despite reference made to the low pitch of esophageal s p e e c h , 2 ' 5 ' 6 · 2 5 ' 2 < " 2 8 ' 2 9 confusions are prevalent in spectrographic findings related to fundamental frequency characteristics of esophageal speakers. lanham and kerr1 3 have suggested that these confusions have arisen from a failure to recognize that a measurable fundamental frequency is often lacking13 in esophageal speech. hence, the need to examine approximate vibratory rates, as opposed to fundamental frequency is apparent. e. other less obvious vibratory features: (i) vibration of the tone is more rapid at the onset of phonation, and slows down as the amount of air in the . esophagus decrease; (ii) the opposite phenomenon to (i) (occuring more rarely10), where subglottic vibrations increase; and (iii) pseudoglottic vibrations may cease for a moment, and then continue at a slower rate than before, thus splitting the word or vowel into two parts.1 0 parameter ii: variables dependent upon degree of neuromuscular control, and affecting periodicity of vibrations: a. sound caused by air intake into the esophagus (swallowing noise): spectrographically the energy in swallowing is fairly evenly distributed over the entire frequency scale. its presence represents what the writer referred to as an 'esophageal click' i.e. one considerably intense pulse (rap) followed by a gap of relative silence prior to the onset of phonation. (see figure 1, 9 and l l i along the horizontal axis). b. leakage of air from the mouth of the esophagus: this may occur prior to and in anticipation of the onset of voicing, and is indicative of an inadequate neuromuscular mechanism (see figure 1, t-5 along the horizontal axis). c. tracheostomal (stomal) noise: this refers to the powerful expulsion of air accompanied by an undesirable murmur from the patient's tracheostoma. strong amplification may occur of those frequencies falling within the bandwidths of the resonators, resulting in horizontal, formant-like bands (see figure 1, a, along the horizontal axis). d. gurgling: this occurs auditorily before, during, a n d / o r after the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) emerging esophageal speech 27 phonation, and is commonly represented spectrographically by highly irregular wide-spaced raps (see figure 2, 22-38 along the horizontal axis). parameter iii: formant structure. a case study was therefore undertaken with the following aims: i. to identify the emergence over time of selected acoustic characteristics which might contribute to a description of the emerging features of esopheal phonation. ii. a further aim was to relate intelligibility ratings of developing esophageal speech to the emergence of these acoustic characteristics. s u b j e c t (s) the s was a 37 year old white portuguese-speaking male. he underwent a total laryngectomy in february 1977 after laryngoscopy biopsy had revealed the presence of widely infiltrating, poorly differentiated squamous cell carcinoma on both his vocal folds. total laryngectomy involved removal of the larynx from above, leaving the esophagus and cricopharyngeal muscles essentially intact. it was not necessary to perform a radical neck dissection. prior to the operation he was a healthy man, with no reported speech or hearing problems. criteria for selection: (a) the s was required to have undergone a laryngectomy operation and to have commenced speech therapy for the first time postoperatively. (b) he was to be capable of producing an esophageal sound involuntarily. this indicates the intactness of his mechanism for esophageal sound production, making an evaluation of his progress from this most basic level possible.3' (c) potential anatomical and physiological variables that could interfere with the s's development of esophageal speech had to be considered. thus, before testing commenced, the ear nose and throat surgeon who performed the laryngectomy was asked questions pertaining to medical information from the questionnaire for biographical, medical, social communication and speech training variables.22 (d) hearing was bilaterally within normal limits. therapy: therapy was administered by a fourth year undergraduate speech and hearing therapy student at the speech and hearing clinic, university of the witwatersrand. over a seventy-five day testing period, the s had received approximately 54 sessions of speech therapy. therapy was directed towards the development of esophageal speech as based primarily upon the work of berlin,3 and snidecor.27 methodology i t a p e r e c o r d i n g p r o c e d u r e s . a. a revox tape recorder was used to produce high quality recordings at random time intervals over a 2xk month testing period. during each recording session the s would read the following data: die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 glenn binder (i) a list of exercises which were constructed by the e. these were based upon snidecor's27 principles of a nine-stage plan of esophageal speech development. (ii) a further group of test words based upon the six groups of consonants found in the portuguese language. (iii) the s used only one overt inflation in repeating plosive syllables (da) as many times as possible. this was based upon berlin's3 measure of mean number of syllables per air charge. (iv) the s used only one inflation in repeating the following two sentences. a. the first sentence contained many liquids: («luisoc iu liunej^nccjeram ocm bcxriocnum o j p i t a j ) b. the second sentence contained an equal number of voiceless plosives: (paula i pejxuj.au muituf kocuj ikocj ojjuj emkasoc) the construction of these two sentences (by the' e), was based upon moolenaar-bijl's16'17 observation that some form of air conservation, or esophageal reinflation is accomplished by plosive consonants. (v) on the final recording session, the s read twenty unseen minimal pair words. b. on one occasion, a normal (laryngeal) portuguese-speaking s read and recorded the same set of stimuli (i) — (v), under the same testing conditions as the s. ii spectrographic/objective assessment: a kay sonagraph model 6061-b (kay elemetrics 6, pine brook, n.j.) was used to produce type b/65 broad-band spectrograms from the tape recordings. selection of speech samples and methods of spectrographic recording: note: several narrow band spectrograms were made of utterances produced by the s on the final session. however, these failed to show harmonic components of the complex speech signals, so that the data in the present study was derived from broad band spectrograms only. for practical reasons it was not possible to spectrographically analyze all the data recorded over the 75 day testing period. thus: (i) ninety-five broad band spectrograms were produced from a list of 19 words recorded by the s at 15 day intervals. these words were selected, as they constituted a representative sample of consonants in the portuguese language (eg. ( p e j ) , (bo'd°c), (lpik«), (doiz), (soblimu)). (ii) 20 broad band spectrograms were also obtained from the s's production of minimal pair utterances on the final recording session. (iii) 39 broad band spectrograms were likewise made of the normal s's recordings of the 19 words, and 20 minimal pair word list. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) emerging esophageal speech 29 iii subjective perceptual assessment: perceptual ratings were made in the following manner: (i) a portuguese-speaking listener who did not know the s, and who was unaware of the nature of the experiment was asked to subjectively rate the intelligibility of all the data recorded by the s at each of the five recording periods selected for analysis. intelligibility of single utterances was determined by the number of correct responses made by the listener to the recorded speech stimuli. intelligibility of sentence material was rated on a five-point equal appearing interval scale. (ii) the ε was assisted by a phonetician in perceptually calculating the following data on each of the five recording sessions: (a) the mean number of plosive syllables (da) per overt inflation in five trials,3 and (b) t h e number of words per breath charge in the sentences constructed."" 17 although these measures were made, together with an evaluation of the relationship between the s's quantified performance over time on skills (a) and (b) above, the findings will not be discussed in the present article. iv phonetic analysis: a phonetician aided the ε in phonetically transcribing the 115 broadband spectrograms. this permitted a direct comparison of unusual auditory qualities perceived by the listener with the spectrographic correlates. results and discussion a. objective data. the analysis of spectrographic data was descriptive in nature. based on the work of several a u t h o r i t i e s 9 ' 1 0 ' " ' 1 3 ' 2 9 the development of the acoustic parameters i — iii and their related variables (described above), were examined in the data obtained in five recording sessions. however, for the purpose of this article, it will be sufficient to document the major changes that occurred in the utterance (bo'd°<) on sessions i and v, thereby highlighting the emergence of these parameters. spectrographic representations of utterances produced by the s on the first recording session (session ϊ) revealed the inadequacy of his neuromuscular mechanism in the following manner: figure i: spectrogram showing utterance (bo d <*): session i. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 glenn binder there was a prominence of random respiratory and tracheostomy turbulence over the whole of the visible frequency scale. resonance amplification created wide horizontal formants with ill-defined edges (see formant 2, figure j, 21-31 along the horizontal axis). the overriding turbulence made clear definition of most utterances virtually impossible. this turbulence appeared to be due to the s's attempts to produce louder, more intelligible speech. this resulted in a burst of outrushing stomal noise frequently louder than the concomitant esophageal voice. most utterances were characterized by several random pulses (raps) before, and immediately after, indicative of swallowing noise caused by air intake into the esophagus. gurgling noises were spebtrographically and auditorily prominent in several utterances (see figure 2 below, 2238, (til)). figure 2: spectrogram showing utterance (til) — gmvling snitnd. in contrast to the inconsistency that was a noticeable feature of the spectrographic displays of the s's utterances on the first recording session, spectrograms of utterances produced on the final recording session (session v) revealed the greatest consistency. this seemed to be due to the s's high morale, in conjunction with increased muscular control and co-ordination over his esophageal air release. figure 3. below reveals the presence of the burping mode of vibration, with considerable energy located in aperiodic components outside the pulses. according to kerr and lanham,1' and kytta,1" this turbulence is due to the probably incompletely interrupted airflow, characteristic of the burping vibratory mode. figure 3: spectrogram showing utterance (bo 'd°<): (session v). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) emerging esophageal speech 31 the s's neuromuscular control still appeared insufficiently adequate and this in relation to an inadequate esophageal airflow, manifested spectrographically as follows: (i) widening of pulses were still evident at the end of utterances, disturbing the continuity of the generally clearly defined formant structure. (see figure 3, 13-14, where the rate of vibration is ~£ 36,99 per second, as compared with around 86,31 per second at 8-9). (ii) apparent air leakage from the esophageal mouth prior to (eg. figure 3, 1-3), and during certain of s's utterances. narrow band spectrograms (eg. see figure 4, below) made of several utterances revealed an absence of harmonic structure and measureable fundamental frequency. this appeared a characteristic feature of the s's low pitched esophageal phonation, which lanham and kerr1 3 have referred to as 'pitchless'. ι yigf figure 4: spectrogram (narrow) showing utterance (pe"). general developmental trends viewed spectrographically several interesting and significant developmental trends appeared to have emerged. the most marked feature throughout was one of inconsistency, postulated to be related partly to the s's neuromuscular, and psychological status at various points in time. a noticeable progression occurred from a rapping to a burping mode of esophageal sphincter vibration in which pulses became significantly more rapid and lower in amplitude. vibrations, however, were conspicuously lacking in the rapidity and periodicity reportedly found in superior esophageal, and laryngeal s p e a k e r s . 1 0 ' 2 0 ' 2 3 ' 2 9 compare the vibratory pattern in figure 3, above, with that in figure 5 below. figure 5: spectrogram showing utterance (bo'd ) of a laryngeal speaker. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 glenn binder generally, spectrographic evidence refuted the concept of fundamental frequency in esophageal phonatitfn. it revealed a vibratory pattern lacking in periodicity, and devoid of a harmonic structure. these measureable rates of irregular vibration appeared to be comparable to what investigators have measured in assessing the fundamental frequency of esophageal speech, «ίο, 11,23,24,» if this is indeed the case, then tato's finding29 that fundamental frequency increases with training, was supported. continuous and more clearly defined formant structure appeared dependent upon a reduction of tracheo-esophageal turbulence, as well as an increasingly regular and rapid vibratory pattern. b. subjective data. listener ratings of intelligibility. table i below reveals an increase in the portuguese-speaking listener's mean intelligibility ratings over the twoand-a-half month recording period. i ii iii iv v carry-over task mean intelligibility rating 1 =20 % 1,5=30% 1,5=30% 3 = 6 0 % 3,5=70% 3,75=75% table i: mean intelligibility ratings as a function of stage of esophageal speech development these findings would appear to indicate the s's progression over the observed period of speech acquisition towards becoming a more proficiently-rated esophageal speaker. the listener experienced the greatest ease in understanding the carry-over task items, corroborating the spectrographically-observed evidence of generalized cricopharyngeal control. comparison of listener intelligibility ratings with spectrographic displays. this revealed the following: i. evidence to suggest a fairly reliable correlation between emerging acoustic characteristics and increasing intelligibility ratings. the latter appeared related to the following acoustic variables: (i) a more rapid and regular burping mode of vibration; (ii) clearer, more continuous formant structure; and (iii) a reduction of tracheo-esophageal noise. the variable (iii) appeared to be the most important independent factor in speech intelligibility ratings, and supported findings reported in the literature. 1 0 ' 1 5 ' 2 3 ii. the s's attempts to produce effective voice-voiceless plosive and fricative distinctions manifested in interesting inconsistent discrepancies between perceptual and spectrographic data. the presence of these the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) emerging esophageal speech 33 discrepancies supported nichol's18 contention that voicing is a vulnerable feature in esophageal speech intelligibility. only two of the numerous discrepancies noted, shall be described in the present article, so as to illustrate the presence of certain phenomena: (i) target: voiceless unreleased plosive (p,t,k) (note: in portuguese, as opposed to english, voiceless plosives are unreleased). word initial position: (n=48) fifteen discrepancies occurred between spectrographic and subjective ratings. the listener heard the powerful voiced cognate predominantly, which was spectrographically represented in the following decreasing order of prominence as a: a. voiceless released plosive. (n= 11) example: (pe j ) — recording session ii (see figure 6, 7) listener's response: (be γ/ p e l ) figure 6: spectrogram showing utterance (pel): (session 11) b. voiced released plosive. (n=4) example: (pipa) — recording session iv (see figure 7, 1-4) listener's response: (bipa/pipa). figure 7: spectrogram showing utterance (pipa): (session iv) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol.25. 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 glenn binder (ii) target: voiceless fricative (s, j) word final position: (n= 10) the listener heard an affricated (j), which was likewise represented spectrographically as a voiceless released affricate. example: (doi j) — recording session iv (see figure 8, 14-17) listener's response: (doit j/doi j) figure 8: spectrogram showing utterance (doij): (session iv) general conclusion from the results, the writer has postulated that the above-described discrepancies, revealing the s's ability to produce perceptually powerful, apparently ejected plosive and fricative consonants, may have been due to a mechanism of pharyngeal compression or 'squeezing'. it is apparent, that the intensity and power with which the s produced these consonants could not have been achieved by the small and variable amount of air reported to be present in the upper esophagus. 1 2 ' 2 7 ' 2 8 rather, the presence of an eggressive glottalic-type of air mechanism, in which a substantial amount of pharyngeal air compression occurred, would appear to facilitate this powerful auditory impression. to produce fricatives, it seemed necessary for the s to block his vocal tract first with a tight labial closure, in order to build up sufficient pressure. affrication resulted both auditorily and spectrographically. this appeared due to the s's inadequate control and consequent inability to produce this articulatory movement of closure lightly. similarly, when the s produced a voiceless plosive, a powerful auditory impression was heard. this was caused by a strong intra-oral pressure bursting through a very tight closure made by the s in an attempt to produce a sufficiently audible voiceless stop. spectrographic evidence likewise supported these above-described findings. for example, when producing medial voicedreleased stops, the 'voice' bar would frequently disappear, and spectrographically a gap would appear prior to the release burst. this gap likewise, may have been indicative; of pressure build-up by the s, to produce an effective auditory impression.12 (see figure 9, 20-22, for an illustrative example). the south african journal of communication disorders vol. 25 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) emerging esophageal speech 35 ' ' h i : · figure 9: spectrogram illustrating mechanism of pharyngeal compression in word-medial position. the only reference to this squeezing-type mechanism appears to have been made by kerr and lanham,9 and pellegrini and raaglini, cited by brooks hunt and va.4 the latter investigators concluded that a squeezing mechanism forces air through a narrow channel, thus producing effective sound. esophageal speech has been described as being a compensation of high d e g r e e . 1 ' 1 0 ' 2 1 this pharyngeal eggressive air mechanism, postulated to be operating, would appear to constitute a compensatory mechanism, rendering support for tikofsky's comment that intelligibility results from modifications 'other than those introduced by the use of a different sound-producing mechanism'.30 i m p l i c a t i o n s observations made in this study suggested the use of a developmental framework to more effectively quantify and assess the patient's process of esophageal speech acquisition. by using serially-made spectrograms, diagnosis and therapy would indeed be interrelated parts of a continuous process of trying to understand an individual and to help him learn.14 the regular clinical use of these objective measures would: (i) provide the ear nose and throat specialist, patient and clinician a meaningful mutual evaluation of the changes in the emergence of esophageal speech. the patient would then be reinforced for intermediary successes, with a concomitant reduction of frustration of having to work towards a remote, sometimes obscure goal of intelligible esophageal speech; and (ii) offer a motivating device which recognizes the need of every patient to gauge his own rehabilitation. in addition, spectrographic data would provide interested family members and friends the opportunity to visualize the patient's progress, and support his effort more realistically. these objective acoustic measures should constantly be compared with subjective perceptual evaluations. therapy goals and tasks would then remain more realistically orientated towards the goal of communicative effectiveness in the patient's linquistic environment, as opposed to an unobtainable level of intelligibility. for example, with regard to the 'vulnerable feature of voicing in esophageal speech', 1 8 constant comparison of objective and subjective data would enable the clinician to assess whether: (i) the patient attempted to produce the voiceless die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings vol.25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 glenn binder consonant and failed to do so audibly, or (ii) whether he omitted it completely. finally, spectrograms should never be used as a substitute for good clinical acumen. judgements in the initial assessment and during the therapy process must be based upon an evaluation of all the parameters available to the clinician, and not on the basis of a single test. however, within these limitations, the use of an analytical, developmental framework would make intelligibility a somewhat more attainable goal for the laryngectomized patient. references 1. bentzen, n., guld, α., and rasmussen, h. (1976): x-ray video-tape studies of laryngectomized patients. j. laryngol. otol., 90(2), 655666. 2. berg, j.w. van den, and moolenaar-bijl, a.j. (1959): cricopharyngeal sphincter, pitch, intensity, and fluency in esophageal speech. pract. orl basel, 21, 298-315. 3. berlin, c.i. (1963): clinical measurement of esophageal speech: (l)methodology and curves of skill acquisition. j. sp. hear. dis., 28(1), 42-51. 4. brooks hunt, r., and va, r. (1964): rehabilitation of the laryngectomee. laryngoscope, 74(1), 382-395. 5. curry, e.t., (1962): frequency measurement and pitch perception in esophageal speech. chapter 5 in speech rehabilitation of the laryngectomized, snidecor, j.c., (ed.), springfield, il.: charles c. thomas. 6. curry, e.t., and snidecor, j.c. (1961): physical measurement and pitch perception in esophageal speech. laryngoscope, 71,415-424. 7. diedrich, w.m., and youngstrom, k.a. (1960): a cineradiographic study of the pseudoglottis in laryngectomized patients. paper read at the ash a convention. 8. hoops, h.r., and noll, j.d. (1969): relationship of selected acoustic variables to judgements of esophageal speech. j. commun. disord., 2, 1-13. 9. kerr, w.a., and lanham, l.w. (1973): anatomical and spectrographic analysis of the voice in disease: a report of five cases. j.s. african speech hear. assoc., 20, 81-107. 10. kytta, j. (1964): finnish esophageal speech after laryngectomy. sound spectrographic and cineradiographic studies'. acta otolaryngol., stockholm, supp. 195, 1-102. 11. kytta, j. (1964): spectrographic studies of the sound quality of esophageal speech. acta oto-laryngol., supp. 188, 371-378. 12. lanham, l.w. (1977): professor, department of phonetics and linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. personal communication. june. 13. lanham, l.w., and kerr, w.a. (1975): pitch in esophageal speech. j.s. african speech hear. assoc., 22, 31-41. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) emerging esophageal speech 37 14. lerner, j.w. (1971): children with learning disabilities. boston. houghton mifflin company. 15 martin, h. (1963): rehabilitation of the laryngectomized. cancer, ' 16(7), 823-841. 16. moolenaar-bijl, a.j. (1951): some data on speech without larynx. folia phoniat., 3, 20-24. 17. moolenaar-bijl. a.j. (1953): the importance of certain consonants in esophageal voice after laryngectomy. ann. orl, 62(4), 979-988. 18. nichols, a.c. (1962): loudness and quality in esophageal speech and the artificial larynx. chapter 6 in speech rehabilitation of the laryngectomized. snidecor, j.c., (ed.) springfield, il.: charles c. thomas. 19. perkins, w.h. (1971): vocal function: assessment and therapy. chapter 20 in handbook of speech pathology and audiology. travis, l.e. (ed.) n.y.: appleton-century crofts. 20. rontal, e„ rontal, m., and rolnick, m. (1975): objective evaluation of vocal pathology using voice spectrography. ann. orl, 84(5), part 1, 662-671. 21. safran, α., and szende, t. (1973): die osophagusprache als sprachlicher kampensations vorgang. folia phoniat., 25, 347-364. 22. shames, g.h., font, j., and mathews, j. (1963): factors related to speech proficiency of the laryngectomized.' j. speech hear. disord., 28(3), 273-287. 23. shipp, t. (1967): frequency, duration, and perceptual measures in relation to judgements of alaryngeal speech acceptability. j. speech hear res., 10, 417-427. 24. sisty, n.l., and weinberg, b. (1972): formant frequency characteristics of esophageal speech. j. speech hear. res., 15, 439-448. 25. snidecor, j.c., and curry, e.t. (1959): temporal and pitch aspects of superior esophageal speech. ann. orl, 68, 623-636. 26. snidecor, j.c., and curry, e.t. (1960): how effectively can the laryngectomee expect to speak? laryngoscope, 70, 62-67. 27. snidecor, j.c. (1962): speech therapy for those with total laryngectomy. chapter 9 in speech rehabilitation of the laryngectomized. snidecor, j.c., (ed.), springfield, il.: charles c. thomas. 28. snidecor, j.c., and isshiki, n. (1965): vocal and air-use characteristics of a superior male esophageal speaker. folia phoniat., 17(3), 217-232. 29. tato, j.m. (1954): a study of sonospectrographic characteristics of the voice in laryngectomized patients. acta oto-laryngol., 44, 431438. 30. tikofsky, r.s. (1965): a comparison of the intelligibility of esophageal and normal speakers. folia phoniat., 17, 19-32. 31. wepman, j.m., macgahan, j.α., rickard, j.c., and shelton, n.w. (1953): the objective measurement of progressive esophageal speech development. j. speech hear. dis., 18(3), 257-251. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) if you don't give them a chance to try the 775 pp what will you say when they find out the facts? hearing aid centre s.a. (pty.) ltd. announces the new dana vox 775pp behind-the-ear model. * 0 c d a t a i n a c c o r d a n c e w i t h u s a s s 3 , 8 1 9 6 7 a n d d i n 4 5 6 0 5 ( p r e v i o u s h a i c d a t a ) : g a i n : o u t p u t : o u t p u t : p 3 o u t p u t : p 2 o u t p u t : p 1 5 8 d b 1 2 9 d b r e 2 0 μ ρ α 1 2 0 d b r e 2 0 μ ρ β 1 1 1 d b r e 2 0 μ ρ β f r e q u e n c y r a n g e : 2 5 0 5 5 0 0 h z a technical novelty is the electric input terminal called the audio-input socket. this audio-input socket facilitates a direct connection to an educational system, fm receiver, television, infra-red receiver or tape recorder employed for teaching or listening purposes. this adaptor which is easily plugged to the lower part of the aid is an optional extra. / available only from: hearing aid centre 212 harley chambers 187 jeppe street johannesburg 2001 tel: 37-2643/4/5 hearing aid centre 633 kerkade centre 267 church street pretoria 0002 tel: 2-6379 & 2-7777 hearing aid centre 104 cartwrights corner cor adderley & darling sts cape town 8001 tel: 22-3665 hearing a i d centre: s u p p l i e r s of h e a r i n g a i d s & allied e q u i p m e n t ; a u d i o m e t e r s ; silent c a b i n s ; e a r p r o t e c t i o n . the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 25, 1978 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a c o m p a r i s o n o f t h e p e r f o r m a n c e o f f i v e a p h a s i c p a t i e n t s o n d i f f e r e n t t e s t s o f l a n g u a g e a b i l i t y b a r b a r a solarsh b.a. (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) summary the performance of five aphasic patients was rated o n three tests of language ability: the minnesota test for differential diagnosis; luria's t e s t s of aphasia; and a test of expressive language based on graded stimuli from the peabody picture vocabulary test. in order to assess communicative-ability of the subjects, each expressive language sample was administered to four judges and a score of c o m m u n i c a t i v e success was c o m p u t e d . t h e study aimed at comparing each subject's performance on these tests of aphasia and its relationship t o the degree of c o m m u n i c a t i v e success, in an attempt t o ascertain which test is the m o s t accurate predictor of " a m o u n t " of aphasic impairment. it also aimed at extracting those variables m o s t useful and appropriate in the diagnosis of the impairment found in aphasic patients. inter-test correlations revealed that tests of aphasia appear to b e accurate predictors of " a m o u n t " of c o m m u n i c a t i v e success. inter-item comparison revealed fourteen sub-tests w h i c h indicated greatest difference in the performance of all the subjects. opsomming i die prestasie van vyf afasie pasiente is met drie t o e t s e van taalvermoe vergelyk. die minnesota t o e t s vir differensiele diagnose, luria se t o e t s e vir afasie en 'n t o e t s vir ekspressiewe taal is gebruik. laasgenoemde t o e t s is o p gegradeerde stimuli van die peabody prent taal t o e t s gebaseer. ten einde die kommunikasie vermoe van die pasiente te bepaal, is elke ekspressiewe taalmonster aan vier beoordelaars oorhandig en 'n telling van kommu'nikasie sukses is bepaal. die d o e l van die ondersoek was o m 'n verband tussen hierdie t o e t s e van afasie en die graad van kommunikasie sukses te verkry in 'n poging o m vas te stel watter t o e t s die mees akkurate voorspelling vir afatiese b e l e m m e r i n g sal bied. dit stel o o k ten doel o m daardie veranderlikes wat nuttig en bruikbaar is in die diagnose van die belemmering in d i e afasie pasient, uit te lig. inter-toetskorrelasies dui aan dat t o e t s e van afasie akkurate voorspellings van die mate van kommunikasie sukses weergee. inter-itemvergelyking dui aan dat daar veertien sub-toetse nodig is o m die grootste verskille tussen pasiente aan te dui. w h e n c o n f r o n t i n g t h e p r o b l e m o f t h e language b r e a k d o w n in a p h a s i a , it falls t o t h e s p e e c h p a t h o l o g i s t t o d e t e r m i n e t h e n a t u r e of t h e p r o b l e m a n d t h e m o s t s u i t a b l e t e c h n i q u e s for r e h a b i l i t a t i o n . it is h e r r e s p o n s i b i l i t y t o select t h e m o s t a c c u r a t e a n d a p p r o p r i a t e m e t h o d of d i a g n o s i s , a n d in o r d e r t o m a k e t h e t h e r a p e u t i c p r o c e s s m a x i m a l l y successful, she m u s t b e fully a w a r e of t h e e x t e n t t o w h i c h t h e s u d d e n l y a c q u i r e d i m p e d i m e n t affects t h e p a t i e n t as a t o t a l a n d c o m m u n i c a t i n g p e r s o n . a n a s s u m p t i o n u n d e r l y i n g "all a p p r o a c h e s t o t h e s t u d y of a p h a s i a , is t h a t it is a d i s t u r b a n c e of language, a n d all i n v e s t i g a t o r s a i m t o study, t h i s a s p e c t . h o w ever, this h a s a f u r t h e r i m p l i c a t i o n . v e r b a l l a n g u a g e h a s b e c o m e m a n ' s p r i m e tdskrif van i suid-afrikanse vereniging virsprak n oorhlknde, vol. , dsmber 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) barbara solarsh means of communication, so that loss of·ability to use language effectively infers loss of ability to communicate effectively. aphasia is therefore primarily a disturbance in the communicative process. this study was prompted by questioning the extent to which the people who treat aphasics, the speech pathologists, are aware not only of the best tests of aphasia, but the degree to which the patient's communication is limited. this implies that it is not only the language impairment that must be studied, but also the communication impairment. consequently both these aspects must be considered in treatment. currently, there are three popular approaches to the study of aphasia: schuell's8 approach, luria's2 approach, and that of the psycholinguists.3 schuell emphasizes the perceptual processes involved in language: luriastresses the highly organized interaction of functional units in the cortical centres of the brain; and the psycholinguists emphasize acquisition of the , rules of language — these approaches are not mutually exclusive, each approach incorporating something of the others. uriel weinreich5 has defined communication as the intentional induction by means of symbols of a certain state in the receiver which corresponds to that, in the sender. thus, successful communication may be regarded as the listener's comprehension of the message as intended by the speaker. even though the language of the aphasic may not be faultless in terms of skills underlying language or in terms of the rules of the language, he may still be able to convey a message. the listener, by having his own intact competence, may still infer and comprehend the intended communication. thus tests of aphasia directed towards the examination of the functions related to language, and to language itself, are in essence evaluating the patient's ability to communicate. the question is to what extent are tests of aphasia reliable as indicators of communicative abilityor " a m o u n t " of aphasic impairment? this study aims at investigating: (i) the relationship between performance on the three tests of aphasia; (ii) the relationship between performance on the tests of aphasia and the patient's ability to communicate his ideas; (iii) it also aims at extracting those items from all the tests which reveal the greatest difference between the subjects on the basis of an inter-item correlation, in an attempt to use them as most accurate predictors of communicative success. method criteria for selection ok subjects: criteria for selection were very broad. (1) each subject had to be diagnosed as aphasic by a neurologist and a speech therapist, i.e. as having suffered damage to that part of the c.n.s. necessary for language, and displaying'evidence of loss of linguistic skills. 1 0 | (2) each subject had to be neurologically stable (the minimum time being six months after onset of the aphasic symptoms). journal of the south african speech (aid hearing association. vol. 21. december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparison of five aphasic patients on tests of language ability 23 (3) each subject had to be regarded as physically healthy and able to cope with the large test battery. subjects five aphasic adults served as subjects. four were males and one was a female; three were right handed and two were left handed.'their age range was,27-70 years, with the mean of 56 years. two of the subjects were university graduates, two had matriculated and one had a standard eight qualification. at present two of the! subjects are working, the other three have no fixed occupation. in four subjects, aphasia was caused by cerebrovascular accidents, and in one subject it was caused by trauma. date of onset ranged from one to ten years, the mean time being four years. diagnosis at the time of onset revealed global aphasia with no peripheral involvement in two subjects, global aphasia with right h'emiparesis in two subjects, and expressive aphasia with right hemiplegia in one subject. amount of therapy received ranged from two months to four-and-a-half years. recent diagnosis by speech therapists indicated predominantly expressive aphasia.in two subjects, predominantly nominative aphasia in one subject, jargon aphasia in one subject, and predominantly expressive aphasia with hemi-paresis and apraxia in one subject. procedure each patient was tested for approximately ten hours over a period of three to four weeks. with two subjects the order of administration was the minnesota test for differential diagnosis (m.t.d.d.a.), the test of expressive language, and luria's tests of aphasia. with three subjects, luria's tests were administered first, and the m.t.d.d.a. was administered last. the order was changed to control for the variable of attitude towards tests affecting test performance in the last tests. it was felt that it would be more suitable to administer the test of expressive language second, to change the nature of testing which was similar for the other two tests. each test session was terminated when the subject began to show signs of fatigue. tests the m.t.d.d.a. the standardised test was administered in accordance with the stipulated test procedure. luria's tests. the principles described by luria2 formed the basis in the construction of the test items. his full battery of tests was administered. where possible exact items were extracted e.g. tests of visual perception, but in most cases the items were devised by the experimenter. test of expressive language. the method described below was selected in accordance with the requirements stipulated by margo e. w i l s o n 1 1 for collecting a language sample. (1) all subjects must be given standard instructions. (2) a standard set of stimulus material must be used that is easily available and convenient to use. picture stimuli from the peabody picture votvdskrif van die suid-afriknanse vereniging vir spraak en gelworheelkunde, vol. 2!, december 1914 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 barbara solarsh cabulary test were used. fifty pictures of graded difficulty along the whole range of the test were shown to each subject. the standardised instruction was: "tell me as much as you can about each picture". each response was transcribed and analysed. a collection of fifty to eighty linguistic responses formed the language sample for each subject. this method was chosen because: (a) it assured the experimenter that a minimum of fifty responses would be emitted. (b) the experimenter would know the nucleus of the target sentence. e v a l u a t i o n οι· communicative success four judges were used to rate communicative success. all the judges were non-aphasic adults with an age range of 25 to 55 years. all judges had matriculation qualifications. all sentences that were to be rated by the experimenter as having one or more errors were read by the experimenter to the four judges in one session. the instructions given were: "if you think you understand the sentence, write what you think that particular sentence is. if you do not understand at all, place a cross next to the number of that sentence". the experimenter made every attempt to reproduce the sentences as the subjects had spoken, considering pause, intonation and gesture in order to convey the full communication as given by the subjects, to the judges. the method was chosen to evaluate the degree to which these aphasic patients could communicate their thoughts about the picture to the judges who were unaware of the original stimuli. scoring m.t.d.d.a. performance was scored according to the criteria as stated in the test manual. luria's tests. criteria for passing or failing were established for each subtest, and performance of each subject was scored according to these criteria. expressive language. each sample was transcribed and total number of sentences was counted. following psycholinguistic principles, each sample was scoredin terms of phonemic, semantic and syntactic errors. phonemic errors were regarded as breakdowns in the use of sounds to d i f f e r e n t i a t e , ^ meanings of w o r d s . 3 these included omissions, distortions, substitutions and additions. a word may contain more than one phonemic error. semantic errors were regarded as the inability to find a word, or inappropriate use of a word. one word may be substituted for another, or a jargon word may be substituted. inappropriate addition of words or jargon was considered as a semantic error. a syntactic error was regarded as any error in the sentence structure caused by incorrect application of rules or omission to apply a rule where indicated. syntactic errors covered the range of transformational rules found within normal language. journal of the south african speech ad hearing association vol. 21 december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparison of five aphasic patients on tests of language ability 25 within each linguistic category scores of one to five were given in accordance with the number of errors in that particular category occurring within the sentence. 5 _ no errors present in the surface structure >> >> >> >> " 4 one error ,, >> " >> 3 _ two errors 2 three errors 1 more than three errors present in the surface structure, but the sentence still conveys meaning. 0 unacceptable sentence. each subject was given a score for phonemic performance, semantic performance and syntactic performance, as well as a total score for expressive language ability. rommunicative success. each sentence proposed by the judges that indicated understanding of the target sentence was given a score of one, and each sentence that was incorrectly understood or not understood at all, was given a score of zero. if the experimenter was unsure of the target sentence, and there was no uniformity in the understanding as portrayed by the judges, the sentence was automatically scored zero. generally, the experimenter was aware of the target sentence owing to prior knowledge of the stimulus, and questioning of the subject at the time the response was emitted. a score of total number of sentences conveying the correct meaning t o the listener was computed as the score of communicative success. inter-item comparison of performance. as the number of subjects in the study was small, statistical inter-correlation was not possible. thus a more subjective method was adopted whereby each sub-test score of one subject was compared to that of every other subject. subjects were compared in pairs, resulting in ten sets of comparisons. the difference between each pair of scores was calculated and the first fifteen scores indicating greatest degree of difference were ranked. each item was then scored according to the number of times it was ranked. the fourteen items selected were those that revealed the greatest differences in more than half of the comparisons made. in all computations scores were computed as percentages in order to provide a standard basis for comparison. results: general comparison of the three test scores: 1. is there any difference between the scores obtained on each of the three tests? a two-factor analysis of variance for repeated measures showed that there was no significant difference between the three sets of scores. (f= 0,3569, df = 2/8, ns). tyskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en geoorheelkunde, vol. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 barbara solarsh test item sub j. 4 sub j. 5 diff. rank luria τ preliminary conversation 41,67 100,00 58,33 14 2. motor functions 58,67 74,67 15,00 3. phonetic anal. & synth. 0,00 92,00 92,00 3 4. writing 64,00 100,00 36,00 5. reading 39,06 90,63 51,57 6. automatic speech 15,52 100,00 84,48 8 7. imitative speech 20,00 89,33 69,33 12 8. nom. function of speech 13,89 100,00 86,11 6 9. predicative function 26,67 86,67 60,00 13 10. grammatic function 0,00 90,00 90,00 5 11. comp. of no. structure 31,43 100,00 68,57 13 12. arith. operations 0,00 100,00 100,00 1 13. phonemic hearing 20,00 94,29 74,29 9 14. word comprehension 30,00 100,00 70,00 11 15. sentence comprehension 45,00 100,00 55,00 16. logical gram. system 26,00 80,00 54,00 17. acoustic motor co-ord. 60,00 86,67 26,67 18. visual perception 63,33 91,67 28,34 19. mnestic processes 25,00 80,00 55,00 20. intellect. processes 0,00 95,71 95,71 2 exp. lang. 1. phonemic performance 7,80 92,80 85,80 7 2. semantic performance . 7,80 98,00 90,20 4 3. syntactic performance 10,60 • 66,00 55,40 15 schuell 1. auditory discrim. 52,14 88,89 36,75 2. vis. & read. disturbances 45,00 93,75 48,75 3. disturb, of numerical rel. & arith. processes 27,27 100,00 72,73 4. sp. & lan. disturbances 27,27 100,00 72,73 10 5. visuo-motor & writing disturbances 51,75 91,23 39,48 / / table i. example of inter-item comparison subjects 4 and 5. 2. is there a general similarity or concordance between the three tests of scores, i.e. are the rankings of subjects fairly similar on all three tests? the kendall coefficient of concordance (\v) showed that there was a significant concordance between the three sets of scores. (w = 0 , 8 5 3 3 , s = 80, ρ 0,01). journal of the south african speech ad hearing association vol. 21 december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparison of five aphasic patients on tests of language ability 3. is there direct correlation between: (a) luria's and schuell's test scores? the spearman rank correlation coefficient ( r s ) showed a perfect correlation of one between the two tests. ( r s = 1,00, n = p 0,01). (b) luria's and expressive language test scores? the same statistical test showed a correlation of 0.825, not significant. ( r s = 0,825, ν = 5, ns). (c) schuell's and expressive language test scores? the same statistical test showed a correlation of 0.825, not significant. ( r s = 0,825, ν = 5, ns). · comparison of r e s u l t s of the t h r e e tests with communication score 1. is there a general similarity or concordance between the three sets of scores as well as those on communication, i.e. between the four sets of scores? the kendall coefficient of concordance (w) showed that there was a significant concordance between the four sets of scores. (w = 0 , 8 6 2 5 , s = 138, ρ 0,01). 1. is there direct correlation between the group's communication scores and their mean scores for the other three tests combined? the spearman rank correlation coefficient r s showed a significant correlation of 0.90 between the communication scores and the combined tests scores. ( r s = 0,90, ν = 5, ρ 0,05). 3. is there direct correlation between the group's communication scores and their scores on: (a) luria test? the spearman rank correlation coefficient ( r s ) showed a correlation of 0,25 which proved not to be significant with only five subjects ( r s = 0,825, ν = 5, ns). (b) schuell's test? the same statistical test showed a correlation of 0,825 not significant. ( r s = 0,825, ν = 5, ns). (c) expressive language test? the same statistical test showed a correlation of 0,800 not significant. ( r s = 0,800, ν = 5, ns). analysis of the three tests for the most reliable items: inter-item comparison revealed the following items as indicators of the greatest degree of difference amongst subjects. tests ranked in nine out of ten comparisons: (1) test of the grammatic system indicated greatest difference amongst subjects l u r i a ' s t e s t tyskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en geoorheelkunde, vol. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 barbara solarsh ss m . t . d . d . a . l u r i a ' s t e s t s e x p r e s s i v e l a n g . c o m m u n i c a t i o n s c o r e 1. 70% 65% 65% 78% 2. 70% 65% 85% 90% 3. 68% 55% 76% 72% 4. 38% 30% 8% 8% 5. 89% 92% 86% 84% table ii. summary of results of performance of subjects on each of the tests. tests ranked in eight out often comparisons: (2) the preliminary conversation — luria's test (3) phonetic analysis and synthesis — luria's test (4) phonemic hearing — luria's test (5) automatic speech — luria's test (6) predicative function of speech — luria's test (7) comprehension of number structure — luria's test (8) arithmetic operations — luria's test tests ranked in seven out of ten comparisons: • (9) imitative speech — luria's test (10) nominative function of speech — luria's test (id. intellectual functions — luria's test (12) disturbance in numerical relations — schuell's test tests ranked in six out often comparisons: (13) word comprehension luria's test (14) syntactic ability — psycholinguistic test discussion comparison of the t h r e e t e s t s of a p h a s i a : results indicate that whereas the tests administered in this study measured different aspects of language breakdown, there was no significant difference in the 'amount' of aphasia as revealed by each test. journal of the south african speech ad hearing associaion vol. 21 december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparison of five aphasic patients on tests of language a b i l i t y 2 9 the significant concordance score verified this by indicating that if a subject performed well on one test, he also performed well on the other tests. inter-test correlations revealed that in spite of different theoretical standpoints, the m.t.d.d.a. and luria's'tests showed a perfect correlation. they were equally reliable in predicting 'amount' of aphasic impairment. correlations between expressive language and each of the other two tests, were both high, although not significant for the small sample. however, the experimenter still feels that it is valid to infer that expressive language ability is reflected in the performance of the other two tests. these correlations may indicate that the contents of the m.t.d.d.a. and luria's tests are more similar. thus the difference in correlation could be explained in terms of 'what' is being tested. this study indicates the importance of both reauditorisation and higher mental functions in the re-acquisition of language, as well as the importance of retrieval and application of the rules of the language. the findings imply that there is a certain element which causes language breakdown in aphasia.it may be regarded as a particular function or a link in a system of functions. however, this element can be quantitatively measured in different ways by the different tests of aphasia. the manifestations as revealed by the various test items may differ, but the central disturbance is consistent. comparison of the t h r e e t e s t s a n d communicative success: 'amount' of aphasia as measured on the tests was shown to concur with 'amount' of communicative impairment, when subjects attempted to convey a message.concerning a particular stimulus picture. assuming that this is reflective of general communicative ability, the speech pathologist may refer to the score obtained by the patient on a test of aphasia as a reliable indication of the degree to which communication is impaired. the experimenter feels that while significant scores may be obtained for communications expressing simple needs or ideas, as the degree of abstraction and complexity is increased, communicative ability of the aphasic would decrease. thus communication at a high level of abstraction would not reflect this high correlation between test performance and communicative ability. however, considering the qualitative continuum of communication, the patient's communicative status may be indicated by the type of sentence he uses. the form of communication he is attempting may be regarded as the aphasic impairment and his ability to convey the message at his own level, as the communication impairment. as previously discussed one may be considered as a product of intellectual ability and the other a product of linguistic ability, although it is always difficult to clearly differentiate between them. a n a l y s i s of the t h r e e t e s t s for the most reliable items: the majority of items from the entire battery were those derived from luria's test items directly related to language and, to auditorisation of sounds, e.g. analysis and synthesis of words and sounds, and phonemic hearing — differentiation amongst sounds and words. auditorisation is implicit in schuell's approach. this supports previous statements validating both theoretical tyskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en geoorheelkunde, vol. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 barbara solarsh i approaches in diagnosis and treatment of aphasia, and emphasises that the approaches are not mutually exclusive. luria's test of the grammatical system, from his tests of expressive language, was revealed to be most indicative of aphasic difference, i.e. it is the best diagnostic tool. this gives support to the psycholinguistic approach to the study of aphasia, of which syntactic performance was ranked as being the most significant of the three psycholinguistic aspects of language. thus ability to apply the rules of the language in both expression and reception, play an important part in a qualitative description of aphasic impairment. intera n d intrasubject comparison of p e r f o r m a n c e : analysis of the performance of each subject on the tests on a subjective basis, has lead to some interesting findings: a comparison of subjects 1 and 2 shows that their performance on the m.t.d:d.a. and luria's tests was similar, but differed considerably in language performance and communicative ability. their levels of higher mental functioning were similar, auditorisation and superimposed perceptual functioning were similar, yet expressive language ability differed considerably. this may be explained in terms of the differing ability to retrieve the rules of the language which supports the psycholinguistic approach to the study of aphasia. it may also be explained in terms of different neurological impairment in the two subjects. a comparison of the performance of subjects 3 and 5 gives rise to different implications about higher mental functioning. for subject 3 the score obtained on luria's tests was the lowest, whereas for subject 5 the score was highest. the influences of premorbid status must be considered as a factor here. although subject 5's communication score was better than t h a i of subject 3, both scores indicated adequate communication on the level that was tested in this study. this indicates that higher mental functionings may not in fact be of such great importance, but with a sample of two subjects, no real conclusions can be drawn. once again, localisation of the lesion may be the crucial factor here. this may be explained in terms of luria's theory by postulating that different systems within the brain were disturbed. although subject 4 could perform certain tasks underlying language as tested on luria's tests and the m.t.d.d.a., his expressive language was extremely poor. his performance was similar to that of subject 5, in that they both scored lower on the language test and higher.on the traditional tests-of aphasia. this supports the idea that there are, in fact, certain skills underlying language, or certain mental activities basic to language which create the foundations of language expression and reception. subjective analysis of the results has indicated that scores obtained on the tests of expressive language were most similar, to scores of communicative success in four out of five subjects. this stresses the importance of a psycholinguistic test in evaluating aphasia. journal o the south african spe n hearing association vol. 21, december 974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparison of five aphasic patients on tests of language ability 31 implications for d i a g n o s i s a n d t h e r a p y : it is a well accepted fact that accurate diagnosis provides the basis to successful therapy. test results reveal a perfect correlation between m.t.d.d.a., and luria's tests. the tests rate equally in their measurement of aphasic impairment with regard to language. the experimenter thus feels that in order to make a full diagnosis of aphasia, the speech pathologist should select either the m.t.d.d.a., or luria's tests and combine this with a psycholinguistic analysis of expression and comprehension. whereas a previous s t u d y 6 has shown that clinical tests are less sensitive than linguistic tests, the results of this study seem to contradict this. the critical element lies in the fact that the subject on which the study was based had minimal language problems. the subjects in this study all had obvious difficulty in language performance. this indicates the relevance of psycholinguistic tests in the final stages of re-acquisition of language. aphasic therapy may be approached from all three different standpoints. the experimenter postulates an eclectic approach to therapy, in which principles of all three approaches are combined. for example, rules of the language may be taught through auditory repetition of examples involving that rule, which includes the basic principle of reauditorisation. in the same way the speech pathologist can help the patient reacquire higher mental functions. mental functions such as memory, can be considered in terms of teaching linguistic units within the limits of the patient's memory span. conclusion the experimenter agrees with goldstein's9 conclusion that "aphasia is no disease, no isolated complex, but it is a functional disturbance of the complex structure of language within the totality of man's capacities and behaviour." in spite of the small sample of subjects, the lack of standardised items for luria's tests, and the subjective analysis of the test items, the study did serve to highlight certain aspects of aphasia. there appears to be a certain element in aphasia which can be measured successfully by the three tests used in the study. aphasia has been considered as causing a defect in communication thus the urgent need to relearn language is directly related to the need to communicate. therefore an efficient mode of diagnosis is essential. basic to a true understanding of the problem of aphasia is an awareness on the part of the speech pathologist as to how great a problem the language impairment really is to the aphasic patient. references 1. critchley, m. (1970): aphasiology and other aspects of language. edward arnold, london. 2. luria, a.r. (1966): higher cortical functions inman. tavistock publications, london. tyilskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en geiioorheelkunde, vol. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 barbara solarsh 3. macmahon, m.k.c. (1972): modern linguistics and aphasia. british journal of disorders of communic., 7, 1, 54-63. 4. mcneill, d. (1966): developmental psycholinguistics. in the genesis of language, smith, f., and miller, g.a. (eds.). m.i.t. press. 5. osgood, c.e. and miron, m.s. (1966): approaches to the study of aphasia. university of minnesota press. 6. penn, c. (1972): a linguistic approach to the detection of minimal language dysfunction. unpublished research report, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. (published in this issue of the j.s.a. speech hearing assoc.) 7. scliuell, h. (1953): auditory impairment in aphasia: significance and retraining techniques. j.s.h.d., 18, 1, 14-21. 8. scliuell. h., jenkins, j.j,, jimenez-pabon, e. (1964)\aphasia in adults. harper & row, new york. . 9. taylor, o.l. (1965): a measurement of functional communication in aphasia. archives of physical med. and rehabilitation. 46j 101-107. 10. tikofsky, r.s. (1966): language problems in adults. in speech pathology. lids. richer. r.w. and brubaker, r.s. chap 11 . northern holland company, amsterdam. 1 i. wilson. m.e. (1969): a standardized method of obtaining a spoken language sample. 7.5\h.r. 12.95-102. journal of the south african speech ai hearing association vol. 21 december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) drooling in cerebral palsy audrey shavell, b.a. logopaedics (witwatersrand) senior speech therapist, forest town cerebral palsy school, forest town, johannesburg summary a review of methods of treatment used over t h e past twelve years in the alleviation of drooling in cerebral palsy is considered. a small group of patients remain persistent droolers in spite of intensive conservative treatment. information was obtained on patients who had been referred for oral surgery between october 1973 december 1976 t o determine whether any common factors would be found among t h e m which might assist in t h e selection of future cases for oral surgery; predict t h e outcome of future surgery more accurately; assess the improvement achieved in t h e t w e n t y cases of this series; and indicate post-operative complications. data was obtained from an analysis of the results of a questionnaire completed by speech therapists o n patients who had all undergone bilateral submaxillary gland excision combined with parotid duct elongation and posterior relocation. the oral surgery was carried out by the late dr. isidore kaplan formerly honorary plastic surgeon, forest town cerebral palsy school. opsomming die kwylprobleem by die serebraalverlamde, en die behandeling daarvan gedurende die afgelope 12 jaar word in hierdie studie in oorsig geneem. 'n klein aantal pasiente hou aan met kwyl tenspyte van intensiewe konserwatiewe behandeling. dit sou dus t o t die terapeut se voordeel strek om meer inligting te he van die pasiente wat gedurende die tydperk oktober 1973 t o t desember 1976 vir mondchirurgie verwys is, om vas te stel of daar enige faktore was wat die pasiente in gemeen gehad het en om dus voortaan keuring vir mondchirurgie te vergemaklik deur 'n meer objektiewe standaard daar te stel en 'n prognose te kan maak. die algemene vordering van die 20 pasiente wat mondchirurgie ontvang het, en enige komplikasies wat ingetree het na die operasie word ondersoek. die inligting vir die studie is verkry deur die analise van 'n vraelys wat voltooi is deur die spraakterapeute wat bogenoemde pasiente behandel het. die chirurgie is uitgevoer deur wyle dr. isidore kaplan, voormalige ereplastiese chirurg, forest town skool vir serebraalverlamde kinders. the problem of drooling in cerebral palsy has troubled therapists working in this field for many years. severe drooling is usually found in those patients who are unable to communicate orally or in those with serious dysarthric bulbar involvements. over the years various approaches have been tried. examples of these are:— 1. constant reminders to patients to close their mouths, suck up and swallow their saliva. 2. mirror work to draw their attention to the escape of saliva and wet chins as-many have, or seem to have, a sensory loss. 3. conscious use of a 'head back' posture, mouth closure and conscious swallowing for increasing lengths of time. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 audrey shavell 4. carefully graded feeding programmes to improve co-ordinated activity of lips, tongue, jaws and cheek musculature on a vegetative level emphasizing posterior placement of food on insertion and insistence on a swallow between each mouthful of food.14 5. hard food programmes.11 6. an adaptation of a device to control extensor thrust of the mandible suggested by westlake and rutherford.16 perspex chin cups were individually fitted and moulded to the shape and size of each child's chin. holes were carefully drilled into the chin cups to allow air to enter and circulate. the chin cups were attached to a headband and when correctly worn facilitated mouth closure and hopefully swallowing. most patients found the appliance very difficult to tolerate. in thefew who were able to tolerate the appliance, it was found that it kept their mouths closed (opposite to their habitual open mouth posture) but did not in any way facilitate swallowing or reduce the pooling of saliva. saliva escaped from the corners of the lips, accumulated in the perspex chin cups and excoriation on the chin usually increased so this was soon abandoned. 7. regular inclusion of sour liquids in the child's daily diet to actually increase salivation and voluntary swallowing. a dessertspoon of lemon juice was given four times a day. the sour taste causes an involuntary sucking in of cheek and lip muscles and a swallow. one patient showed a definite improvement in control of drooling for 15-30 minutes after drinking the lemon juice but then complained of stomach pains. control of drooling regressed to the pre-treatment state after the initial state of improvement. 8. encouraging the child to have all liquids and semi-liquid foods (like soups, porridge, custard) through a plastic tube or straw to improve functioning of lip musculature and better co-ordinate sucking and swallowing movements. 9. teaching the child to chew chewing or bubble gum, under supervision, to increase awareness of oral muscle functioning and develop mouth' closure. 10. operant procedures to eliminate drooling in one cerebral palsied adolescent were successfully used by garber.5 11. mueller's application of neuro-developmental principles to speech therapy, and specifically her approach of trying to achieve saliva control through feeding and drinking therapy, has been extensively used since early 1974 λ her approach is based on normalizing muscle tone throughout the body. particular attention is paid to head, neck and trunk position while muscle tone is reduced in oral areas (in the case of the spastic) and more normal swallowing patterns are facilitated. normalizing the response to intra-oral, digital stimulation and facilitating mouth closure forms part of helen mueller's pre-speech therapy programme. in all phases of her 'mouth' therapy she frequently interrupts the particular activity to facilitate swallowing. she suggests south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) drooling in cerebral palsy 77 various specific methods to overcome drooling by developing mouth closure and eliciting a swallow which all staff members should include in their handling of the patient. the child's attention is never drawn to the drooling as mueller believes it overburdens the child to expect him to consciously think of swallowing. 12. sensory stimulation techniques icing, brushing and resistance techniques applied in and around the mouth to increase sensory awareness, improve mouth closure and swallowing in carefully selected patients.13 the latter two methods (oral stimulation and sensory stimulation) have been most successful when combined with graded feeding programmes in those patients treated from a very young age on neuro-developmental lines. however, even in some of-these cases, when they begin walking or the amount of walking increases, a deterioration in control of drooling occurs,10 and one has to reintroduce drooling therapy. control of drooling may also break down when the child is involved in fine motor activities/classwork etc. 13.' medical treatment used has been atropine type drugs to actually reduce the flow of saliva to try and 'dry up' these patients. these proved to be unsuitable because of the relatively large doses required and the side effects. other medical and surgical approaches reported in the literature include division of the auriculotemporal nerve and submandibular ganglion of the tympanic plexus this works temporarily but drooling recurs. sectioning of the chorda tympani nerve may result in loss of taste sensation, though it is claimed that this eventually does return.4 diamant and kumlein3 excised the sub-mandibular gland on one side and the chorda tympani on the contralateral side within the middle ear. the results of surgery on 12 mentally retarded cerebral palsied patients aged 3 1 7 years over a two-year period, revealed that five patients were free of symptoms, three patients showed considerable improvement and four patients showed slight improvement. parotid duct ligation works, but has a high incidence of refistulization and parotitis.1 worster drought19 described various medical and surgical procedures used with patients suffering from suprabulbar paresis who were severe droolers. these are illustrated in his description of the treatment of case vii, 'j' an eight year old child diagnosed as a case of congenital suprabulbar paresis who was a severe drooler with extremely dysarthric speech. an attempt was made to relieve the persistent dribbling by prescribing atrophine sulphate (1/100 grain three times daily) but the result was only a slight and temporary reduction in the degree of dribbling. tincture of stramonium (up to 30 minims every four hours) produced a similar result. it was then decided to try the effect of x-ray irradiation to the parotid glands on both sides. j. was given daily treatment in hospital for one week, and for about six weeks afterwards there was slight improvement in the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 audrey shavell degree of dribbling, probably because less saliva was produced. however, several weeks later dribbling was as profuse as before. the following year bilateral ligation of stensen's duct from inside the cheek was performed, but again the result was not satisfactory and there was only slight improvement of the dribbling. one year later an examination of the parotid ducts was carried out by probing. on the left side the ligature was found to be adequate, but on the right side stensen's duct was found to be patent throughout its length. the left submaxillary gland was then removed. the operation resulted in considerable improvement in dribbling. by the end of each morning there were only a few stains on his shirt-front, compared with the previous wet 'dickie' area. dribbling occurred only when close mental concentration made him forget to swallow excess saliva. by the beginning of the following year there had been a further improvement in dribbling and he now had to change his shirt during the day only twice a week, compared with twice a day prior to operation. towards the end of the year there was some relapse in the degree of dribbling, and the right submaxillary gland was removed. after this last operation dribbling became negligible. our experience has been that if the child has not acquired control of drooling by the time he reaches the age of formal schooling (between the age of six and eight years), he is usually very distressed by the constant soiling of school books, papers, equipment and table-top activities and usually resents having to wear bibs. in desperation we have suggested the use of cotton towelling wristbands to dab wet chins as this may be more socially acceptable for the older child. many of the children have wet chins and sodden top clothes and require several changes of clothing a day. depending on how fastidious the parents are, we have known children who have had 2 8 3 0 bib changes a day in an attempt to keep them dry. the social stigma of drooling increases as the child gets older and parents and siblings find it increasingly difficult to be 'loving' and 'patient' with the drooling member of the family although the patient may be unaware of his drooling on a physical level he is made aware of it by the looks and stares of strangers and the remonstrations of those in the immediate family circle. the social and economic problems faced by the older adolescent and young adult drooler were vividly brought to our attention early in 1973 when we were asked to assist an intelligent, 21 year old, self-conscious, spastic hemiplegic female who had very severe involvement of oral musculature and who had no control of drooling. habitual mouth position was open and isolated movements of lips, tongue and jaw were virtually impossible. any attempt to move parts of the speech mechanism (e.g. prptrude or elevate her tongue) or to vocalize or communicate orally, caused a marked increase of tension in face, jaw, cheek and neck muscles, accompanied by a strong tempero-mandibular joint slip. she used a spelling board toispell out individual letters in communicating. basic functions of chewing, sucking and swallowing were very poor and there was much loss of liquid in drinking. she drooled continuously; on effort, amount of drooling was increased. she was living in a south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) drooling in cerebral palsy 79 centre for adult cerebral palsied patients in natal where her occupation was to assist in the kitchen. the inability to control her drooling was both a threat to her job and a serious embarrassment to her. there appeared to be no automatic swallowing pattern and pooling of saliva was so excessive, and oral musculature so severely impaired that prognosis for acquiring control of drooling using conventional methods was considered to be hopeless. the possibility of oral surgery was considered and fortunately, at the time, a paper by brody became available. in his paper, brody1 acknowledged that the pathophysiology of drooling and the principles of its surgical correction were first described by theodore wilkie,1 7' 1 8 a canadian plastic surgeon. wilkie noted the significance of tongue function in the control of saliva while observing cineradiographs taken to analyse the palatal movement of cleft palate patients. he concluded that swallowing was divided into oral and pharyngeal phases. in the normal person in the oral phase of swallowing, the co-ordinated tongue first manipulates the food and/or saliva onto it and then forms a cup which seals against the hard palate sweeping the bolus posteriorly and depositing it into the pharynx. this initiates the second phase, the pharyngeal phase which is automatic. when one considers that twenty-one muscles are used in a split second during a swallowing movement and that the average person swallows approximately two thousand times a day, one appreciates how difficult it is for the cerebral palsied child to manipulate the saliva and/or food back into position for swallowing. the cerebral palsied child's tongue discoordinately slaps against the palate, sending the oral contents in all directions with only a portion reaching the pharynx. brody1 assumes that the pharyngeal phase of swallowing in cerebral palsy is relatively normal, if the child has had no respiratory or nutritional difficulties, and that the main cause of drooling is the ineffective tongue co-ordinating mechanism. approximately 1,000 to 1,500 cc of saliva are produced daily. 90 to 95% of saliva is produced by the submaxillary and parotid glands while the remainder is derived from buccal and sublingual glands. the submaxillary glands empty into the mouth through ducts located under the tongue anteriorly in the floor of the mouth and the parotid ducts emerge in the upper cheeks at the level of the second maxillary motor. two or more salivary glands can be removed with relative ease, but parotid resection is more complicated with considerable risk to the facial nerve.7 in 1967, wilkie17 postulated that if the saliva could enter directly into the pharynx, the need for tongue manipulation would be by-passed, sufficient saliva would remain for mastication, pooling of saliva in the front of the mouth would cease and drooling should be controlled. working on the basis that a strip of mucosa could be buried submucosally and would spontaneously tube itself, he lengthened the parotid duct and rerouted it into the tonsillar fossa. in this way the saliva from the parotid secretions by-passed the tongue and were deposited far enough posteriorly for the pharyngeal phase of swallowing. this substantially decreased the amount of saliva in the anterior portion of the mouth. wilkie first described two patients who were successfully treated for drooling by the above described surgical procedure,17 and later reported die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 0 audrey shavell a series of eight patients treated in a five year period including his two original cases.18 seven of the eight cases required a second surgical procedure viz. submandibular gland resection. significant improvement in control of drooling was recorded in all eight patients. brody1 reported on a large series of over sixty patients, aged between 6—65 years, who had the combined procedure of bilateral submaxillary gland resection and posterior transplantation of the parotid duct. approximately onehalf of these patients had a primary diagnosis of severe mental retardation (iq range 15 — 60) with or without cerebral palsy. the other half of his series were primarily cerebral palsied with lesser degrees of intellectual impairment. of brody's first 60 patients, 55 had sufficient improvement in their drooling to consider the surgery successful. of this group, approximately two-thirds had no residual drooling and minimal or no side effects. stenosis of the orifice of the new duct occurred in 13 patients representing 16 of 120 ducts. brody1 stated that recent refinement of the operation has lowered the incidence of stenosis. the surgical procedure described by wilkie17 was employed by dr. isidore kaplan to successfully alleviate our young adult's drooling problem. subsequently nineteen other patients, ranging in age from 4 years 9 months to 21 years, had the bilateral procedure of submaxillary gland excision with parotid duct elongation and relocation. fourteen patients were males and six were females, and all had been diagnosed as uncontrollable droolers who had not responded to conservative measures.7 the remainder of this paper consists of a follow-up report on dr. kaplan's' series of cases based on the results of a preand post-operative oral surgery questionnaire completed by speech therapists working at cerebral palsy schools in the republic of south africa. it was hoped that analysis of the results of the questionaires would provide us with additional information on the problem of drooling in cerebral palsy. information was obtained on nineteen of the twenty patients who underwent oral surgery between october 1973 and december 1976. as this is a relatively small sample, detailed statistical analysis of the results was not undertaken. the writer has attempted to determine response trends in possible relationships between severity of drooling and the following factors:(a) type of cerebral palsy. (b) degree of involvement. (c) intelligence. (d) presence of a complicating oral sensory loss and/or apraxia. . (e) habitual mouth position and swallowing pattern. (f) oral communicative ability. (g) length of period of pre-operative speech therapy. on the post-operative side we were interested in comparing: (a) the surgeon's evaluation of results in each case with the; speech therapists' rating of the success of oral surgery and whether these assessments agreed with the parsnts' opinions of the results of surgery, (b) the incidence and types of post-operative complications, (c) the incidence and types of post-operative behavioural south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) drooling in cerebral palsy 81 disturbances. many of these children are subjected to several orthopedic surgical procedures during their formative years and the psychological effects of additional surgery should not be overlooked.12 patients attended the following cerebral palsy schools or adult cerebral palsy centre. name of school no. of patients case no. as in table i forest town cerebral palsy school 5 4 , 5 , 1 0 , 1 1 , 1 2 . nuwe hoop cerebral palsy school 1 1 muriel brand cerebral palsy school 2 8,15. pretoria cerebral palsy school 2 9,14. west rand cerebral palsy school 6 2 , 6 , 7 , 1 3 , 1 6 , 1 7 . pevensy place adult cerebral palsy centre 1 19 two patients were not attending cerebral palsy schools and no questionaires were completed on them. information on these two patients was obtained from the surgeon's files (case numbers 3 and 8 as listed on table i). case no. age at surgery yrs mths type of cerebral palsy degree of involvement most recent iq assessment 1 4 9 athetoid and spastic quadriplegia c a . 4,7; m a . 7 , 1 l-reynell verbal comprehension μα. 2,2. pp.v.t. (peabody) 2 7 aphasia {dysarthria no phys. inv. 1q 69-snijders-oomen non verbal intelligence scale. 3 6 not known not known not known 4 7 6 spastic quadriplegia 1q 61-old sa.1.s. iq 48-new s.a.i.s. 5 7 9 spastic, pseudo bulbar palsy, apraxia bilateral \ hemiplegia/ 1q 52-stanford binet; iq 56-columbia 6 11 spastic and athetoid quadriplegia iq 77-snijders-oomen non verbal intelligence scale. 7 11 athetoid quadriplegia iq 60-columbia mental maturity scale. 8 11 8 athetoid not known iq 107-new s.a.i.s. 9 11 8 spastic and athetoid quadriplegia iq 85-columbia mental maturity scale. 10 12 7 spastic bilateral \ hemiplegia/ iq 83-new s.a.i.s. 11 12 11 spastic bilateral \ hemiplegia / iq 62-new s.a.i.s. 12 13 3 spastic athetoid quadriplegia iq 55-pj.v.t. (peabody) 13 14 3 spastic quadriplegia iq 64-columbia. 14 is spastic quadriplegia iq 50-ravens progressive matrices. 15 16 7 spastic quadriplegia iq 73-new s.a.i.s. 16 17 spastic quadriplegia iq 73-ravens progressive matrices. 17 17 2 spastic hemiplegia iq 86-old s.a.i.s. 18 18 spastic quadriplegia not known. 19 21 spastic hemiplegia not known. table i summary of selected features from the case histories of the droolers. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 2 audrey shavell s έ = 8 2s » 9 ο oo j s s o α u ο g * " £ < ο μ μ st o o c l o o v o o o o o o o o o o o ^ l o z 2 z 2 z > z z z z z ? z z z z z z z o o ^ o o w o o « o o o o o o o o ¥ . o zzzzz>*zz>*zzzzzzzzzz v) m m α>α><̂ 00α> oovoouvc.ovoc.a» ο « ϋ α> ο a) α>4>4>4> ο ο α> α> α>^.α> z>-z>->-z>->->->->-zz>-z>->-z>· « 3 3 ο 4> ·—. β : ζ >• ζ £ . μ ϊ έ . η μ . μ η μ μ η μ 10 ^ μ ου'— ο υ υ ο υ υ υ υ ο υ υ υ ο^,υ ζ>·ζ.ζ.>>·ζ.>>·>>ζ.>>·>·*. ζ ζ >• ζ + + ζ + + + + 1+ + + ζ μ ζ u + + + + + ζ + ^ ι ϊ ^ ϊ , ϊ . ι * s* * s s« ζ ο ζ ζ-3 ο ζ ζ ζ ζ ο ζ ο ζ ζ ο ο ζ ζ 6 : 3 j ο "53 α 2β ο χ α. c c c c c c c c c c ' z o o o o o o o o o o o o o o o u o o c c c « c c « c c c c c c c c c c c c α α α ο α α ο ααααα.αο.ο.αο.αα ο ο ο υ ο ο υ ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ututut g * * α> ω-—.-—.-—. ω . ο . ω ο ω —. ζζζζζ>>·ζζζ>·ζζζζ>ζ>·ζ t i s ο 9> s ι -a a + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + + ε ί i ι ι h " .s s-s e.s "•s ii 3 c is i i . s f § s f l o 2 >· •3 1 i f o, λ ut » « ό α> i, .. χ ο = * h i j s ω ο -ο 75 c c· .ο ο « ο ji ut a. -s -· &. 00 ; » σ* ! i s s g e ι2 — α> ·η e = η *o ' s u. ω ζ ; ί ι , · = χι ο·«! » u 2 3 i § 1 ? ϊ κ u 8 » ϋ λ -ο i s 2 . i 3 3: «β α> ο 1 | | : ° ο ο όι α> ο 1 ί ϊ ! i f δ 8 i s • — <λ ι-, ^ ω ,j? ι 8 (μ f « ο i i 1« c « . ε gju a •3 oil=· μ >, υ •a 3 1 = jj • i f . e l s 5-36 «ρ ό 5 c α \ j « or o, i u o · · ϊ » : « c j < ο : ο ο ut t3 <υ ο ό <υ u <υ ut . 3 τ3 <υ ο & λ 3 c/i ω j η c η 77if south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) drooling in cerebral palsy 83 table i indicates that the majority of severe droolers are spastic or mixed spastics and athetoids. only two patients were diagnosed as athetoids and one as predominantly athetoid with some spasticity. when one considers degree of involvement, the majority of patients who are severe droolers are the physically more severely affected ones, i.e. quadriplegics or double hemiplegics (involvement of all four limbs, arms more than legs and involvement of bulbar musculature.) there was only one patient in the series who had no physical involvement, two were hemiplegics and two whose degree of involvement was not known. it is difficult to make firm statements about intelligence because of the variety of intelligence tests used. in the sixteen patients where some formal assessment was available iq scores ranged from 50 — 107. twelve of the sixteen patients had intelligence quotients below 80. table ii shows that fourteen of the nineteen patients were rated by the speech therapists as continuous droolers, requiring several changes of clothing or bibs each day. three were rated as severe droolers not necessarily requiring change of clothes or bibs. two were rated as moderately severe droolers i.e. drooling could be controlled while concentrating on it but control broke down when the child engaged in other activities. the answer to the fourth question, whether the child drooled while sleeping, was in most cases not known. habitual mouth position at rest and on effort was open in the majority of cases as was to be expected. mouth position while sleeping was not known in twelve of the nineteen patients. as far as pre-operative awareness of drooling was concerned, nine patients were classified as being acutely aware of their drooling, five had some awareness and three were totally unaware of their drooling. the speech therapists felt that eleven of the patients had a possible oral sensory loss, five had no oral sensory loss, no information was available on two cases and a therapist was not sure whether there was an oral sensory loss in one case. when swallowing ability is considered, we see that eleven of the nineteen patients had problems projecting food from the front to the back of the mouth. once the bolus of food reached the pharynx twleve of the nineteen subjects could swallow normally, four had problems in the pharyngeal phase of swallowing as well. no information was available on swallowing on the remaining three patients. ten of the nineteen patients had some or much loss of food while eating. two patients frequently regurgitated their food and one patient had problems of liquids and semi-liquids being expelled through the nose. table iii does not contain information on years of pre-operative speech therapy and frequency of treatments per week is not set out in detail as the number of years of pre-operative therapy varied between one and ten years and number of weekly sessions between one and five. a wide variety of methods and techniques was used so meaningful comparisons cannot be made. results of table iii indicate a complicating apraxic component in seven out of the sixteen cases on whom questionaires were completed, i.e. an incidence of 43% — a higher incidence of apraxia in a cerebral palsied group than one normally expects to find. fifty per cent of the patients were unable to communicate orally and among the remaining 50%, half had very distorted speech with poor speech intelligibility and the other half were patients with articuladie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 84 audrey shavell rating scale of case severity of no. apraxia dysarthria/apraxia method of communication. 1 2 3 1 no + points. vocalizes. simple gesture. 2 yes + + ++ distorted oral communication. 3 n/k + + + ++ not known. 4 no communicates orally. good intelligibility. 5 yes + eye pointing. communication board. 6 yes + gesture & typing. 7 yes + communication board. 8 no + distorted oral communication. 9 no + gesture. communication board and typing. 10 no . + very distorted oral communication. 11 no communicates orally. reasonable intelligibility. 12 yes + gesture. vocalisation. communication board. 13 yes + gesture. communication board. 14 no + isolated vocalisations. types. 15 n/k + very distorted oral communication. 16 yes + gesture and typing. 17 no + communicates orally but distorted. 18 n/k not known. 19 no + joints. simple spelling board. legend: + + = excluded in totals. 0 = communicates orally. reasonably-good intelligibility. n/k = not known. rating scale of severity of dysarthria/apraxia. 1) totally unable to communicate orally. / 2) attempts to communicate orally, but severely distorted. 3) communicates orally but has articulatory + phonatory errors. table iii summary of selected features related to the speech of the patients. tory and phonatory errors. one child (case no. 4) had speech completely within normal limits although she was a severe drooler and another child (case no. 11) has intelligible speech after several years of speech therapy. south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) drooling in cerebral palsy 85 table iv consists of a comparison between the surgeon's evaluation of results achieved and the various speech therapists' ratings of the success of the surgical intervention. details of post operative complications and the parents' evaluation of surgery are also included. case no. surgeon's evaluation of results post operative comnegative psychological speech therapists' evaluation of parents' opinions plications reactions results a b c d a b c d 1 + none none + very pleased. 2 + none none + not satisfied. 3 + +++ none n/k very pleased. . 4 + + +++ none none + satisfied. 5 + none none + satisfied. 6 + none n/k + n/k. 7 8 + + none none none y e s + + + very happy. happy. 9 + stenosis of none + happy. 10 + +++ left duct none y e s + + + + happy. 11 + stenosis of none + very happy. 12 + right duct none y e s + + + + + very satisfied. 13 + none n/k + satisfied. 14 + none none + expected more. 15 + none none + n/k. 16 + +++ none none + n/k. 17 + none none + satisfied. 18 + n/k n/k + very pleased. 19 + none none + satisfied. l e g e n d : n / k — n o t k n o w n . + + 3 cases w e r e e v a l u a t e d b y t h e s u r g e o n as b e t w e e n g o o d a n d e x c e l l e n t a n d h a v e b e e n t a b u l a t e d a c c o r d i n g l y . t h e y w e r e n o t h o w e v e r c o u n t e d t w i c e i n final t o t a l s . + s o m e b e h a v i o r a l d i s t u r b a n c e s w e r e n o t e d p o s t o p e r a t i v e l y , t h e s p e e c h t h e r a p i s t felt p r o b a b l y m o r e d u e t o m a r i t a l strife in t h e h o m e . + + + w e e p y a n d i n s e c u r e for o n e w e e k after d i s c h a r g e f r o m h o s p i t a l , h a d b e e n fine w h i l e in h o s p i t a l . + + + + v e r y u p s e t for a b o u t o n e m o n t h p o s t o p e r a t i v e l y t h e n s e t t l e d d o w n . table iv summary of the evaluation of selected features of surgery. examination of table iv shows exact agreement between the surgeon's evaluation of results and the various speech therapists' ratings in ten of the eighteen cases i.e. 55%. the surgeon did not rate any case as representing no improvement, i.e. drooling problem unchanged, but the speech therapist and parents gave one case this rating. (the surgeon evaluated this case as fair). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 6 audrey shavell four more cases were rated as being fair by the speech therapists than by the surgeon. twelve of the nineteen patients achieved an excellent result according to the surgeon and four a good result. the speech therapists' figures for these two categories were five and five respectively. stenosis occurred in only two of the thirty-eight ducts operated on i.e. 5,2% of cases. no instances of bilateral stenosis occurred. occurrence of stenosis was not related to whether the parotid ducts were tubed or not.7 this is a lower incidence of stenosis than that reported by brody1 where stenosis occurred in 13,3% of cases. three of the fifteen patients on whom information was available had negative psychological reactions to the oral surgery. none of these lasted longer than one month. fourteen of the sixteen parents were satisfied or highly delighted with the results of oral surgery, one set of parents were dissatisfied, one expected more and information was not available on three cases. additional positive results were mentioned on two of the cases. in case i, the speech therapist commented on the fact that the child had an immediate increase in appetite and a gradual disappearance of halitosis which had been a problem pre-operatively. one other case was reported as being very thirsty pre-operatively but not post-operatively. results and conclusions the majority of droolers in this study were spastics or spasticity was the dominant disability when more than one disability was present. as was to be expected, most of these patients were severely involved in terms of degree of disability. seventy-five percent of them had intelligence quotients below eighty as assessed on various scales. habitual mouth position was open in seventeen of the nineteen patients on whom questionnaires had been completed. an oral sensory loss was suspected in eleven of eighteen patients and seven out of sixteen or 43% were considered to be apraxic in addition to their severe dysarthria. fifty percent of the group were totally unable to communicate orally, twenty-two percent could communicate verbally but with very poor intelligibility and the speech of the rest of the group ranged from reasonable intelligibility to normal speech. there was a fifty-five percent agreement in rating improvement between the speech therapists and the surgeon. agreement in rating improvement between the therapists and the parents was higher. only one child was considered by the speech therapist and his parents as totally unchanged post-operatively and as a fair result by the surgeon. in all the other patients, improvement in control of drooling ranged from fair to excellent. there has been a very low incidence of stenosis and fistula formation which kaplan attributed to the method of creating a vshaped, rather than a circular, orifice of sufficient width in the tonsillar fossa.7 study of the four cases where the pharyngeal phase of swallowing was considered abnormal by the speech therapists reveals that three out of the four had a poorer surgical result than the rest of the group. conclusions cannot be south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) drooling in cerebral palsy 87 drawn from such limited data but more detailed pre-operative assessments of swallowing including cineradiography will be done on future patients. this may enable us to predict more accurately the kind of results to expect from oral surgery and be able to advise parents accordingly. it is also incumbent upon the speech therapist to inform both the patient and parents about the post operative hospital treatment which includes administration of fluids and antibiotics intravenously for the first forty-eight hours, intra-oral suction and routine oral hygiene and the restriction of intake to only clear fluids. on the third day a soft diet is permitted with mouth washes and continuous oral hygiene. frequent encouragement to swallow by the nursing staff is important and regular visits by the speech therapist help to maintain the morale of patients and family. most patients are discharged on the seventh or eighth day. all patients have fairly marked facial oedema for a while post-operatively. adequate pre-operative preparation for hospitalization reduces the likelihood of psychological trauma occurring post-operatively. oral surgery is not suggested as a panacea for all cerebral palsied droolers. the twenty cases who underwent the surgical procedure described in this paper represent a very small percentage of patients in cerebral palsy schools in the republic. it is suggested as a form of treatment which should be offered to persistent droolers who have not shown improvement with other methods. the positive psychological effects observed post-operatively in nineteen of the twenty patients and their families were quite dramatic. many of the older patients actually verbalized that the drooling was the most distressing and unpleasant feature of all their disabilities. persistence of drooling into late adolescence and early adulthood is accompanied by self-consciousness and feelings of inferiority and inadequacy which increase problems in job placement, even in sheltered workshop situations. acknowledgements the writer gratefully acknowledges the interested assistance of dr. g. brody, rancho los amigos hospital, california for introducing us to the field of oral surgery and to the late dr. i. kaplan who carried out the oral surgery. his patience and kindness towards both patients and paramedical colleagues will always be remembered. sincere thanks to the speech therapists who completed the questionnaires from which the data was drawn. references 1. brody, g. s. (1972): the surgical control of drooling in cerebral palsy. paper presented to the american academy for cerebral palsy 2. brody, g. s. (1973): the surgical control of drooling in cerebral palsy. abstract of paper presented to american academy for cerebral palsy. devel. med. and child neurol, 15, no. 2. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gg audrey shavell 3. diamant, m. (1974): a treatment for drooling in children with cerebral palsy./. laryngology otology. vlxxxviii, no. 1. 4. enfors, b. and lundberg, a. (1969): diebehandlungder hyper salivation bei kinder in mit zerebral paresen. paper presented at a meeting, dusseldorf. 5. garber, ν. b. (1971): operant procedures to eliminate drooling behaviour in a cerebral palsied adolescent. devel. med. child neurol., 13,641. 6. kaplan, i., and shavell, a. (1974): the surgical control of drooling. film/slide demonstration and presentation to cerebral palsy division of the national council for the care of cripples in south africa. june 25th, president hotel, johannesburg. 7. kaplan, i. (1977): results of the wilkie operation to stop drooling in cerebral palsy. plastic and reconstructive surgery. 59, 5, ρ. 646. 8. mueller, h. (1972): facilitating feeding and pre-speech. in physical therapy services in the developmental disabilities. pearson, p. et al. (eds), c. c. thomas. springfield, 111. 9. mueller, η. (1974): handling the young cerebral palsied child at home, w. heinemann. med. books 2nd. ed. 10. mueller, h. (1976): notes from speech therapy workshop. forest town cerebral palsy school, july 1976. 11. palmer, m. (1947): normalization of chewing, sucking and swallowing reflexes in cerebral palsy: a home programme. j. speech dis., 12, no. 4. 12. reynell, j. (1965): post operative disturbances observed in children with cerebral palsy. devel. med. child neurol. 7,360-376. 13. rood, m. (1969): sensory stimulation techniques. notes from the rood course. s.a. physiotherapy soc. 14. shavell, a. (1973): home speech therapy programme for the young cerebral palsied child. forest town cerebral palsy school. 15. shavell. a. (1977): changing attitudes and approaches to speech therapy with cerebral palsied patients. s.a cerebral palsy j. 21. 16. westlake, h. and rutherford, d, (1961): speech therapy for the cerebral palsied. national soc. for crippled children and adults, inc. 17. wilkie, t". f. (1967): the problem of drooling in cerebral palsy a surgical approach. canadian j. surg., 10:60. 18. wilkie, t. f. (1970): the surgical treatment of drooling: a follow up report of five years experience. plastic and reconstruction surgery. v, 45. no. 6. 19. worster drought, c. (1974): suprabulbar paresis. devel/med. child neurol, 16,1. supplement no. 30. south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological and neuroanatomical findings in three cases with apraxia of speech* lesley wolk ma (speech pathology) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract this study investigated whether or not the articulatory errors on single consonants and initial consonant clusters in three adult aphasic patients with apraxia of speech could be characterized by underlying phonological processes. naming tasks and spontaneous narrative tasks were used for elicitation of speech samples. computerized tomography was carried out in order to determine possible neuroanatomical correlates of the linguistic findings in these cases. results revealed that in each case, it was possible to trace underlying phonological processes which fit into strategy groups operationalizing particular phonetic preferences. similar trends occurred on all tasks. self-employed strategies to aid phonological production were used by all cases during their struggle with successive attempts towards the target utterance. neuroanatomical findings demonstrated brain lesions which were more extensive than would be expected in the traditional concept of a focal lesion in broca's area. in each case the left parietal region was involved. theoretical and clinical implications are discussed. opsomming hierdie studie ondersoek die moontlikheid dat onderliggende fonologiese prosesse die artikulasie foute (enkel konsonante en inisiele konsonant kombinasie) van drie afasiepasiente met verbale apraksie kan karakteriseer word. benoemingstake en spontane verhalende spraak is gebruik om spraakmonsters te ontlok. gerekenariseerde tomografisie is onderneem om moontlik neuroanatomiese korrelate van linguistiese bevindings in hierdie gevalle te bepaal. resultate dui daarop dat dit in elke geval moontlik was om onderliggende fonologiese prosesse uit te wys. hierdie prosesse kon ingedeel word in groepe wat spesifieke fonetiese voorkeur verleen t.o.v. strategic soortgelyke neigings het in al die take voorgekom. selftoegepaste strategies om fonologiese produksie te bevorder, is deur alle pasiente gebruik tydens hulle worsteling met opeenvolgende pogings tot die teiken uiting. neumanatomise bevindings het getoon dat brein letsels groter was as wat verwag word van die tradisionele konsep van 'nfokaleletsel van broca se area. in elkepasient was die linkse parietale area betrokke by die letsel. teoretiese en kliniese implikasies is bespreek. ι controversy has further ensued regarding the nature of articulatory impairment. some investigators have argued that articulation errors are random and unrelated to the target sound (critchley, 1952; deal and darley, 1972) while others have concluded that errors are systematic and rule-governed (blumstein, 1973; martin and rigrodsky, 1974a; 1974b; klich, ireland and weidner, 1979). the development of apparent incompatible conceptions of the disorder is due, at least in part, to the phenomenon itself. in addition, guidelines for treatment have been somewhat fragmentary and meagre. numerous workers in the field of adult apraxic disorders have extrapolated from child language in an attempt to unravel some of the conceptual controversies discussed and to design effective treatment programmes. there may be several! imitations of this procedure, for example that of over-simplifying the adult aphasic breakdown. yet, the knowledge acquired through extrapolating from child language has contributed in some way * this study formed part of the author's ma dissertation entitled "phonological impairment in aphasia" submitted to the department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31 1984 @ sasha 1984 recent research into the area of aphasia has witnessed a surge of interest in articulatory disturbances, particularly in broca's aphasic patients. multiple terms have been employed to describe the articulatory impairment; for consistency in this paper, the term 'apraxia of speech' will be used. the investigation of apraxia of speech in this study extracts from linguistic approaches and neurological methods of investigation. apraxia of speech is both controversial and complex despite many years of research. several investigators have described the disorder as a motor programming disturbance not primarily due to neuromuscular or higher language functioning deficits, and have suggested that the programming of phonemes can be selectively impaired without impairment of language (deal and darley, 1972). martin (1974), however, objected to the term 'apraxia of speech' and argued that motor programming disturbances are inextricably tied to language deficits and cannot be defined except within the framework of aphasia. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological and neuroanatomical findings in three cases with apraxia of speech 37 to the understanding of these cases and to the overall clinical management. research on distinctive feature analyses in patients with apraxia of speech suggests that error productions are systematically related to their target sounds in terms of a feature breakdown (martin and rigrodsky, 1974a; 1974b). there has been limited application of phonological process analyses (bowman, 1982). phonological processes, as they are described in child language, refer to general simplifying processes that affect entire classes of sounds (ingram, 1976). they do not reflect random errors, rather they are said to result from a set of systematic strategies operationalizing a child's phonetic preferences (ingram, 1976; 1981). it has been argued that the phonological processes may provide the general patterns that need to be eliminated in therapy, and that if trained on key sounds, generalization is likely to occur (weiner, 1979). cohen, gelfer and sweet (1980) feel that it is essential for the clinician to evaluate the aphasic patient's speech performance and to compare this with the anatomical localization afforded by computerized tomography (ct). buckingham (1979) further argues that it is difficult to determine what constitutes an apraxia of speech without further specifying the location of the lesion and the stimulus conditions which evoked the behaviour said to be apraxic. thus the primary goal of this study was to investigate whether the articulatory errors on single consonants and initial consonant clusters in three adult aphasic patients with apraxia of speech could be characterized by underlying phonological processes, considering which ones operate most commonly; a secondary goal was to describe ct scan findings in these cases. these findings are discussed briefly in a recent report (wolk, 1982a). method subjects three adult male aphasic patients were used as subjects for this study. all cases were chosen from johannesburg hospitals and were diagnosed as aphasic by neurologists and speech pathologists. they ranged from age 58 to 73 years with a mean age of 63,3 years. all cases presented a history of left (l) hemisphere cerebrovascular accident (cva). they demonstrated various degree of concomitant broca's aphasia. in each case, however, phonological deficit predominated. they ranged in months post onset (mpo) from 9 to 24, with a mean of 15,7 months. all cases were native speakers of english and had no previous history of speech or hearing problems, premorbid psychological problems, senility or mental retardation. years of education ranged from 8 to 11 with a mean of 9,7 years. case 1 had the most severe form of apraxia of speech, and case 3 the least severe disorder. subjects had to have suffered a left (l) cerebral infarct, and were required to have reached a point table 1 description of cases age sex aetiology months post onset mpo type of aphasia home language educational level premorbid occupation handedness speech therapy received case 1 (a.m.) 58 male cva 24 broca english std. 6 plumber (r) 18 months case 2 (ft:) 73 male cva θ broca english std. 8 despatch manager (r) 4 months / /case 3 (e.j.) 59 ma e cva 14 broca english std. 9 salesman (r) 12 months table 2 results of preliminary tests ! / preliminary test case 1 (a.m.) case 2 (f.t.) case 3 (e.j.) boston diagnostic aphasia examination (bdae) goodglass and kaplan (1972) score on severity rating scale = 2 score on severity rating scale = 2 score on severity rating scale = 2 token test de renzi and faglioni (1978) 34/36 = 940/0 34/36 = 94o/0 3 6 / 3 6 = 100% oral non-verbal gesture battery moore, rosenbek & la pointe (1976) 56/56 = 100% 56/56 = 100% 56/56 = 100% screening pure-tone audiometric test normal normal except 45db at 4000 hz bilaterally normal goldman-fristoe test of auditory discrimination (goldman, fristoe and woodcock, 1970) 100% 100% 100% discrimination of cvc and ccvc words (designed by the writer) 100% 100% 100% die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 lesley wolk of neurological stability considered as six months after the neurological insult (sarno and levita, 1971). the description of cases is summarized in table 1. procedure preliminary tests were carried out to determine whether the subjects satisfied the selection criteria. these are summarized in table 2. two approaches were used for elicitation of speech for linguistic analysis: — (1) naming tasks and (2) spontaneous narrative tasks. 1. naming tasks a test comprising two parts was developed by the writer for use in this study. part i was designed to test single consonants in initial (i) and final (f) word position, and part π was designed to test initial consonant cluster sequences. criteria for selection of words part i of the articulatory test was composed of 89 items, carefully selected to elicit all the consonants (c) in english in (i) and (f) word positions. an attempt was made to select items in which the desired consonant in (i) and (f) word position could be elicited simultaneously, thus maximally conserving the total number of picture stimuli required (for example, initial id/ is elicited by the picture words 'dog' and 'duck' at the same time that final igl and /k/ are being elicited). each consonant was elicited at least twice in (i) and (f) word position, ranging between two and four different phonemic contexts. the phonemic environment was varied with respect to both vowels and consonants in the immediate phonemic vicinity. thus 81 words represented consonants in (i) position, and 70 words represented consonants in (f) position, while the total number of test items was equal to 89. part π of the articulatory test was composed of 99 items. each (i) cluster of english was elicited in at least two stimulus words, ranging between two and four different phonemic contexts. experimental manipulation of words stimulus items were constrained by the following:' i) phonological rules governing the combination and sequencing of phonemes in english; ii) words which could be pictorially represented, for the reason that imitation was to be deliberately avoided; iii) word length and syllable structure were considered mono syllabic and disyllabic words were included, while multisyllabic words were excluded; iv) morphological endings were excluded when devising the list of single consonants in (f) position so as to elicit 'pure phonological constructions'. the entire articulatory test thus comprised 188 stimulus items (part i = 89; part ii = 99). each response was elicited twice, to provide an estimate of the subjects' consistency of production of the same sounds in identical environments. thus both parts of the naming task, naming singletons (ns) and naming clusters (nc) were administered twice to all subjects. 2. spontaneous narrative tasks these included an open-ended conversation, description of fifty pictures and constructing a story from six pictures depicting real-life scenes. administration of phonological assessment procedure all testing took place in a sound-treated room and was carried out by the writer during several 45-minute sessions. subjects were tested individually. for the naming tasks, pictures were presented in a random order, but attempts were made not to present two words with the same consonant or consonant cluster successively. part i (ns) and part ii (nc) were administered twice over four sessions, following the same procedure. for the spontaneous tasks, each subject had to describe the fifty picture cards selected and to tell a story for each of the six picture cards chosen. spontaneous narrative tasks were administered once over three or four sessions. response transcription all responses were recorded on a high quality revox tape recorder (model 1132 dolby version). wherever possible onsite transcriptions were carried out by the writer, utilizing both visual cues and contextual information. tape recordings were transcribed by the tester in broad phonetic transcriptions, and were used as a comparison with on-site transcriptions. a second transcription of all data was carried out using three transcribers who served as judges. judges worked independently and then met jointly to resolve any discrepancies, discarding any instances in which no consensus could be reached, providing maximum objectivity and accuracy of recorded data. phonological process analysis the goal was to discover whether articulation errors of these subjects could be characterized by general processes, and if so, which phonological processes were operating for each case. all cvc and ccvc words with error productions were analyzed in terms of one or more of the following broad categories as described by weiner (1979): a) syllable structure processes, b) harmony processes and c) feature contrast processes. within these categories, several specific phonological processes were selected from those described in child language (ingram, 1976; 1981; weiner, 1979). i it was possible that multiple processes could occur at the same time for a particular error utterance, in which case credit jwas given to as many processes as were operating for that utterance. in this way it was possible to determine the frequency of occurrence of each process and its probable resulting effect on intelligibility. the same procedure was carried out for both naming and spontaneous tasks for each case. assessment procedure for neurological investigation neuroanatomical investigations of the cases in this study included general neurological examinations carried out by the same neurologist, and computerized tomography (ct) scans for each case. ct scans were performed with contrast enhancement on the elscint 905 system. ct scan pictures obtained from the assessment procedure for the cases of this study were interpreted by a radiologist and two independent neurologists in order to provide a measure of objectivity and reliability. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological and neuroanatomical findings in three cases with apraxia of speech 39 results and discussion phonological findings tkble 3 represents a summary of the phonological processes operating for all cases. the findings indicate that in each case certain phonological processes occurred more frequently than others, and that these occurred in similar proportions on both naming and spontaneous tasks. the findings will be discussed below in terms of three broad categories, i.e. syllable structure processes, harmony processes and feature contrast processes. syllabi" structure processes the three most commonly used syllable structure processes for all cases, were deletion of final consonant, weak syllable deletion and cluster reduction. cluster reduction was the most commonly operating phonological process on naming tasks for all cases. it was necessary to develop a subcategory of the cluster reduction process to indicate that it was possible for an error substitution to reflect an extraction of select features of one or more elements of the target segment which merge together to form a new segment. this was referred to as 'feature merging'. feature merging comprised a substantial number of errors, for example in the case of a.m., 27,64% and 34,48% of cluster reduction errors were seen to occur on naming and spontaneous tasks respectively. an example of feature merging is clearly illustrated in the error substitution /sp/ for the target segment /skw/ which occurred in cases 1 and 3. here it can be noted that isl remains the same, /p/ retains the plosive quality of c2 but acquires the labial quality of c3 in the target segment. a review of the data indicated that there was a frequent collapse of 2-element and 3-element cluster segments into one element in all cases. in a.m.'s system, the sound preference or neutralization process (weiner, 1979) was /(/ as in j/sl, |/sk, ]/spl, (r/skw. in f.t.'s system, there were two sound preferences, /]/ and if i as in j/skw, j/str, f/skw, f/spl, f/bl, f/gr, fr/skw, fl/spr. in e.j.'s system there were three sound preferences /(/, if i and isl as in j/str, j/skw, f/)r, f/kw, s/0r, s/fr s/tw. therefore the blade-palatal fricative / / was a favourite sound strategy in all cases, the labiodental fricative if i was common to f.t. and e.j., and the tip-alveolar fricative isl was an additional favourite sound strategy in e.j.'s system. the processes of reduplication, cluster contraction and epenthesis were operating in all cases, although less commonly table 3 a distribution of errors on cvc and ccvc words according to phonological processes no. and % of errors naming spontaneous phonological process a.m. f.t. e.j. a.m. f.t. e.j. no % no % no % no % no % no % syllable 1. deletion of final consonant structure 2. weak syllable deletion processes 3. cluster reduction feature merging 4. reduplication 5. cluster contraction 6. epenthesis / vowel / consonant 49 5,94 32 3,88 123 14,93 (34)* (27,64) 1 0,12 6 0,73 24 2,39 (11) (45,83) (13) (54,17) 51 12,17 35 8,35 89 21,24 (14) (15,73) 7 1,67 0 , 0 10 2,39 (2) (20,00) (8) (80,00) 13 5,78 4 1,78 39 17,34 (8) (20,51) 1 0,44 1 0,44 6 2,67 (4) (66,66) (2) (33,33) 40 6,23 66 10,28 58 9,04 (20) (34,48) 4 0,62 2 0,31 24 3,74 (7) (29,17) (17) (70,83) 46 9,74 71 14,61 40 8,23 (7) (17,50) 4 0,82 3 0,62 7 1,44 (2) (28,57) (5) (71,43) 14 4,64 30 9,93 48 15,89 (3) (6,25) 1 0,33 5 1,66 9 2,98 (3) (33,33) (6) (66,66) i harmony 7. assimilations processes a) progressive (p.a.) b) regressive (r.a.) initial consonant devoicing final consonant devoicing prevocalic voicing labial assimilation alveolar assimilation velar assimilation nasal assimilation 8. metathesis feature metathesis 29 3,52 62 7,52 15 1,82 16 1,94 9 1,09 21 2,55 41 4,98 13 1,58 10 1,21 30 3,64 (13) (43,33) 18 4,30 28 6,68 4 0,96 13 3,10 13 3,10 6 1,43 30 7,16 2 0,47 2 0,47 11 2,63 (7) (63,64) 11 4,89 27 12,00 3 1,33 7 3,11 4 1,78 14 6,22 13 5,78 2 0,89 2 0,89 1 0,44 (0) (0) 22 3,43 50 7,79 10 1,56 4 0,62 7 1,09 21 3,27 36 5,61 3 0,46 9 1,40 16 2,49 (8) (50,80) 26 5,35 33 6,79 12 2,47 6 1,23 11 2,26 14 2,88 30 6,17 2 0,41 6 1,23 12 2,47 (9) (75,00) 13 4,31 29 9,60 8 2,65 2 0,66 3 0,99 4 1,33 31 10,27 2 0,66 2 0,66 6 1,99 (1) (16,67) feature 9. stopping : contrast 10. fronting , processes 11. denasalization 12. nasal substitutions 13. gliding ' 14. velar substitutions 15. liquidation 16. interchange of /i/ and iii 17. frication 18. affricatian 19. vocalization (weakening) 66 8,01 93 11,29 12 1,46 27 3,28 16 1,94 23 2,79 27 3,28 49 5,94 4 4,9 26 3,16 0 0 9 2,15 29 6,92 2 0,47 7 1,67 4 0,96 5 1,19 11 2,63 12 2,86 4 0,96 8 1,91 9 2,15 14 6,22 28 12,45 2 0,89 5 2,22 5 2,22 4 1,78 6 2,67 1 0,44 5 2,22 5 2,22 2 0,89 47 7,32 103 16,04 17 2,65 13 2,03 9 1,40 7 1,09 29 4;52 36 5,61 4 0,62 4 0,62 1 0,16 22 4,53 80 16,46 9 1,85 10 2,06 4 0,82 8 1,65 10 2,06 6 1,23 5 1,03 0 0 9 1,86 19 6,29 44 14,57 5 1,66 3 0,99 3 0,99 2 0,66 8 2,65 5 1,66 2 0,66 1 0,33 3 0,99 total i 824 100 419 100 225 100 642 100 486 100 302 100 * those, numberscenclosedibetween brackets are sub-divisions of major categories and are therefore not included in the totals of each column. die-suid-afrikaanse jyuikrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 lesley wolk than those processes discussed above. reduplication reflects the patients' awareness of the number of syllables in a given word, though he may not have the ability to produce the variety of syllables in sequence. for epenthesis in all cases, the insertion of a consonant was usually slightly more common than the insertion of a vowel for both tasks. an observation of the data showed that in vowel epenthesis, the vowel most commonly used in all cases was the mid schwa /a/, whilst in consonant epenthesis, a.m. used nasals, stops and liquids, f.t. and e.j. used nasals, stops, liquids and fricatives. cluster contraction was used less commonly. an observation of the data showed that when it was employed however, it was more likely to result in a voiceless rather than a voiced affricate. the common use of cluster reduction in aphasic patients with apraxia of speech is well documented in the literature (blumstein, 1973). the interesting observation in the present study was the finding that when clusters were reduced the error segment could reflect an extraction of select features of one or more of the target elements, a phenomenon referred to in this study as 'feature merging'. feature merging occurred in each case, but was found to be most common in case 1 (the most severe case), and least common in case 3 (the least severe case). this would seem to imply a simultaneous processing (decoding and encoding) of features comprising different phonemes. further evidence is provided by the observation noted during testing that in each case occasional error utterances seemed to reflect the simultaneous production of two elements of a cluster. the intrusive schwa comprised the major part of vowel epenthesis, a process commonly reported in previous reports (klich et al., 1979). blumstein (1973) observed epenthesis of schwa and stop consonants, and she feels that these errors are often motivated by the particular environment in which they occur. harmony processes harmony processes were generally common in the system of these cases. assimilations occurred more commonly than metatheses and in each case regressive assimilations (anticipatory errors) outnumbered progressive assimilations (post-positioning errors) on both naming and spontaneous tasks. for a.m. and e.j. regressive assimilation occurred twice as frequently as progressive assimilation, while the difference was slightly narrower for f.t. it may be observed in table 3 that there was a striking predominance of alveolar assimilation on both tasks for all cases. there was a prevalence of alveolar and labial assimilation for a.m. and e.j. velar and nasal assimilation occurred less commonly. other assimilatory processes such as prevocalic voicing, initial and final consonant devoicing occurred, though less commonly in all cases. assimilations took place not only with respect to syllables, phonemes or distinctive features within the boundary of a word, but also to those across word boundaries. most errors occurred within a word. metathesis occurred commonly in a.m.'s system, less commonly in f.t.'s system and was uncommon in e.j.'s system. a subcategory of metathesis was developed to indicate a reversal of individual features as opposed to reversal of an entire phoneme, which will be referred to as 'feature metathesis'. feature metathesis comprised a major part of the total number of errors reflecting metathesis in the cases of a.m. and f.t., while only one instance of feature metathesis was noted on spontaneous tasks in the case of e.j. in f.t., for example, 63,64% and 75,00% of metathesis errors were seen to occur on naming and spontaneous tasks respectively. the general trend that regressive assimilation occurred more commonly than progressive assimilation and that metathesis was less predominant, is consistent with previous findings (blumstein, 1973; itoh, sasanuma and ushijima, 1979). by contrast sasanuma (1971) found metathesis to be more common than assimilation, but it is of interest to note that he found a similar occurrence of feature metathesis in his patients. the findings of feature metathesis contributes to the viewpoint discussed above that these patients may have simultaneous processing (decoding and encoding) of features comprising different phonemes. according to itoh et al. (1979), the coarticulatory phenomenon in aphasic patients, especially anticipatory coarticulation, can be regarded as some manifestation of cortically generated motor programming. the analysis of data from a coarticulatory point of view thus seems to indicate that some 'future scanning' appears to be operating in these patients with apraxia of speech, so that for example, the articulatory gestures for the following segments are brought into the preceding segments. anticipations, perseverations and transpositions of speech sounds occur in normal speakers and have been referred to as 'slips of the tongue' (fromkin, 1971). feature contrast processes several processes governing the substitution of one sound for another were operating in each case. it may be observed in table 3 that fronting and stopping were clearly the two feature contrast processes most commonly used, where fronting was more common than stopping. fronting and stopping were predominant for a.m. and e.j., yet fronting alone was most common for f.t. other processes operating for all cases were denasalization, nasal substitutions, velar substitutions, liquidation, interchange of /]/ and /r/, frication, affrication, vocalization and gliding. a review of the data showed that in the process of gliding, the substitution of /w/ was generally more common than /j/ in all cases. i these findings are consistent with previous research (critchley, 1952; blumstein, 1973). specifically, the prevalence of stopping, in patients with apraxia of speech, has beein highlighted (klich et al., 1979). in support of the universality of language dissolution, peuser and fittschen (1977) found some similar phonological processes, such as stopping and denasalization, to be operating in a case of a turkish aphasic, and thus feel that "phenomic disorders due to aphasia seem to have a regular and universal character" (peuser and fittschen, 1977, p. 202). thus in common with blumstein (1973), it can be concluded that many of the error types found in these aphasic adults with apraxia of speech, do in fact reflect the natural phonological processes which operate in all languages. in this study the same phonological processes were seen to be operating for both singletons (cvc) and clusters (ccvc), providing evidence for the systematic jgovernment of articulation errors. blumstein (1973) showed that phonological error patterns were systematic within groups and were similar across groups of aphasic adults. it might be valuable to investigate further whether phonological errors in other aphasic types (with varied lesions) y the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological and neuroanatomical findings in three cases with apraxia of speech 41 could be characterized by similar natural phonological processes as found for the broca's aphasic cases of this study. the data in this study refer to the speech samples of three adult cases. they therefore clearly cannot form the basis for any extensive comparison with the development of speech in children. however, the findings suggest that there are some differences between the phonological dissolution in these apraxic cases and phonological acquisition in the child, but that the general patterns in these adult cases resemble those used by children. the reader is referred to wolk (1982b) for a further discussion. during the course of eliciting phonological data it became clear that all cases tended to employ a number of different strategies to aid phonological production. self-employed strategies were used on naming and spontaneous narrative tasks in their struggle with successive attempts towards the target utterance. the following strategies will be discussed below: 1. revisualization of the written word. 2. use of semantic associations. 3. silent articulations. 4. reduced speech rate. 1. revisualization of the written word case 1 (a.m.) and case 2 (f.t.) were frequently able to evoke clear-cut mental images of the written words and could operate with these mental images, while they were completely unable to articulate them. a.m. could spell the appropriate letter(s) of a target word out aloud, although he could not produce them phonically, for example "y" for "yacht", "g" for "frog" and "c,q" for "squirrel". it may be noted that he arbitrarily selected a letter from the initial or final word position. furthermore, he spelled out aloud the letters "s", "k", "y" for the word "sky", but could not articulate the word as a whole. he was often noted to recall the number of letters comprising a target word, such as "three letters" for "zoo". on one occasion a.m. was seen to perform the mechanical movements of writing with his j fingers in an attempt to spell the word, i.e. "x, y, ζ . . . zed" for "zoo". it thus appeared that he was mechanically converting sound sequences into graphic symbols. j f.t. was also noted to spell the words put aloud in order to facilitate articulatory production. the interesting feature noted in this case was that the articulatory error production often reflected a revisualization of the visual representation of the word (the graphic symbols) rather than the phonic representation (the acoustic image). for example /i d t j . . . j dtj/ and /i dtj . . . t| d:/ were successive attempts at the target word "yacht". it can be observed here that the error utterances show possible signs of interference from f.t.'s visual associations with spelling pronunciation variants. the finding that these cases recall printed letters in order to aid articulatory production is consistent with previous literature (nebes, 1975). 2. use of semantic associations all cases were seen to employ a carrier phrase with semantic relevance in order to aid articulatory production. this tendency was the most prominant in f.t. table 4 provides an example from the data for f.t., illustrating how he uses several semantic associations in order to recall the target item 'zebra'. here it can be seen that some basic features of the object to be named are distinguished. buckingham and rekart (1979) state that this necessitates a clear image of the object on the one hand, and a well-organized acoustic-phonologic structure of the appropriate name on the other. for further discussion, refer to wolk (1982b). table 4 an example of the use of semantic associations to recall the target item 'zebra' by f.t. target word error utterances in the sequence as produced by c2 zebra /z zlfva zfba blfva taijki hd:s r . . . . straips/ 3. silent articulations it was observed during elicitation procedures that all cases used subvocal activity to aid articulatory production. this was in the form of continually moving articulatory structures groping to find correct postures. on presentation of a stimulus item a.m. and e.j. were sometimes noted to monitor themselves silently using covert articulations prior to producing the word aloud. similar behaviour was seen for f.t., although his covert articulations seemed to be combined with an audible whisper, and it was noted that for f.t. the labiodental fricative /f/ was commonly used during repeated trials of subvocal activity. an interesting observation was that f.t. often achieved correct articulation during whispering but not with voicing. this would suggest that one of the manifestations of the difficulty in this case with apraxia of speech, might be the co-ordination between articulatory and phono-respiratory mechanisms. these findings are consistent with studies reported in the literature pertaining to both normal subjects and cases with apraxia of speech (mcguigan, 1970; warren, 1977). mcguigan (1970) demonstrated that in normal adults internal verbal processes (rehearsal) are usually accompanied by small, covert movements of the lips, tongue and larynx which may be referred to as 'implicit speech'. mcguigan (1970) further states that as the performance of a task becomes more automatized, both the amplitude and the frequency of occurrence of these movements tend to decrease, which led to the suggestion that covert oral activity plays an important role in language. nebes (1975) in common with warren (1977) contends that patients with apraxia of speech have relatively intact implicit speech patterns. for the cases in this study, it is interesting to note that the amplitude and frequency of covert articulatory movements were significantly less in e.j. than in a.m. and f.t., suggesting that in the more severe cases of this study, automaticity of the articulatory movements was more impaired. in an attempt to extrapolate from various proposals put forward by nebes (1975), the following theoretical possibilities can be considered: (a) whether covert oral activity in these cases may in feet fecilitate processing of articulatory images in the presence of possible auditory feedback disturbance, and die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 lesley wolk (b) it may be that proprioceptive/kinaesthetic feedback from the muscles of articulation, which in these cases was felt to be disrupted (noted on oral stereognostic findings, which are not discussed in this paper), is an important factor in formulating an internal representation of the articulatory form. 4. reduced speech rate finally a most significant observation was that all cases, particularly a.m., tended to reduce speech rate during their struggle with successive attempts towards the target utterance. this together with their deliberate syllabification of words was seen to aid correct production. this finding seems to highlight the importance of temporal sequencing in articulatory production and has obvious clinical implications. neuroanatomical findings the ct scans for each case are presented below (see figures 1, 2 and 3). figure 1 case 1: a.m.'s ct scan showing cerebral infarction involving mainly the (l) parieto-occipital region and extending towards the island of reil (insula) on the (l) side. figure 2 case 2: f.t. 's ct scan showing cerebral infarcts in the (l) parieto-frontal region involving broca's area and in the (r) parieto-occipital region. figure 3 case 3: e.j.'s ct scan showing cerebral infarction involving the (l) parietal region extending towards the'vertex and interiorly to the (l) island of reil (insula) and upper temporal lobe. table 5 summary of ct scan results for all cases case 1 a.m. case 2 f.t. case 3 e.j. | (l) parieto-occipital region, extending towards the island of reil (insula) on the (l) side a) (l) parieto-frontal region including broca's area b) (r) parieto-occipital region c i (l) parietal region, extending towards the vertex, interiorly to the island of | reil (insula) and upper temporal region on the (l) side moderate degree of cerebral atrophy generalized cerebral atrophy ct scan asymmetry of lateral ventricles (left larger than right) asymmetry of lateral ventricles, where (l) lateral ventricle is more dilated than (r) ventricle widening of sylvian fissure on (l) side generalized dilation of ventricular system, and widening of sulci over the vertex widening of sylvian fissures bilate, rally (left more than right) / no displacement of midline structures no displacement of midline structures no displacement of midline structures .. ^ no abnormal enhancement occurred after contrast some peripheral enhancement of lesion in (l) broca's area occurred after contrast no abnormal enhancement occurred after contrast / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological and neuroanatomical findings in three cases with apraxia of speech 43 the results will be discussed in relation to current research in the neuroanatomical correlates of language. each case discussed presents with the major neurological insult in the dominant (l) hemisphere. in the cases of a.m. and e.j., the major lesion is in the (l) hemisphere. in the case of f.t., the major lesion is in the (l) hemisphere with a minor lesion in the (r) hemisphere, which is said to have resulted from a mild transient (r) hemispheric cerebral embolus suffered previously. . . . a major function of the left hemisphere is the control of changes in limb or articulatory posture, and its complex verbal and praxic functions are derived from such control. (kimura, 1977, p. 540) here kimura (1977) suggests that the (l) hemisphere contains a system specialized particularly for producing the correct articulatory posture, and consequently also for controlling the transition from one position to another. halsey, blauenstein, wilson and wills (1980) studied regional cerebral blood flow measurements in normal speakers and speakers with brain lesions. they found that in normal speakers during verbal activation, there were significant increases above rest level in the inferior frontal region, roughly corresponding to broca's area. there were no significant differences between (l) and (r) hemisphere regions. according to halsey et al. (1980), this relative symmetry of response suggests that probably relatively little intellectual effort is involved in ordinary speaking, and that the bilateral inferior frontal principle activation mainly reflects bilateral cortical control of the muscles of articulation. further measurements were made during attempted speaking in patients with (l) and (r) hemisphere lesions. in the (l) hemisphere lesion group, the largest flow changes were in the inferior frontal and temporal regions bilaterally. the only interhemispheric significant difference was in the parietal region in the (l) hemisphere lesion group. these findings are of particular interest in relation to the neurological findings of the cases in the present study. it may be observed that ct scans for ea£h case revealed involvement of the parietal region. x i many years ago liepman forecast that lesions permitting the appearance of apraxia ('facial apraxia' and 'apraxic dysarthria' now referred to as 'apraxia of speech') would be found in the parietal lobes, in the preand post-central gyri and the foot of the first two frontal gyri, with the subjacent part of the centrum semi-ovale, and the corpus callosum (nathan, 1947). according to buckingham (1979), the supramarginal gyrus is a crucial zone for the discussion of apraxia in general and of apraxia of speech. he states that: although it is a cortical area of the parietal lobe strategically located for the language zones, it is also anatomically quite near the arcuate fasciculus fibers traveling through opercular regions. (buckingham, 1979, p. 212.) lebrun, buyssens and henneaux (1973) describe a' single case with 'pure anarthria' who suffered a vascular accident in the (l) parietal lobe. the patient presented clinically with apraxia of speech, an absence of bucco-facial apraxia and no paralysis. in a more recent case study, investigations of an apraxic patient showed a lesion in the (l) parietal region (di simoni and darley, 1977). itoh, sasanuma, hirose, yoshioka and ushijima (1978) reported on ct scan findings in a patient with apraxia of speech, indicating an infarct involving the cortical surface near the anterior tip of the sylvian fissure of the (l) hemisphere and the immediately subjacent white matter. f.t.'s ct scan revealed a well marked lesion in broca's area on the (l) side. evidence in the literature suggests that a lesion in broca's area will result in disintegration of skilled coordinated movements of the articulatory organs, referred to as motor apraxia of speech (whitaker and seines, 1975; mohr, pessin, finkelstein, funkenstein, duncan and davis, 1978). it is probably not possible to identify a unitary aspect of speech with a delimited cortical area in the narrow sense, given the present state of our knowledge and methodology. while broca's area is not felt to have a unitary function, there is recent evidence to support the general hypothesis that broca's area is specialized for certain expressive (motor) components of language, particularly speech or articulatory parameters (whitaker and seines, 1975). goodglass and geschwind (cited in whitaker and seines, 1975, p. 92) have stated that: . . . injuries in this area (broca's area) appear to implicate most directly but not exclusively the phonological and graphic aspects of language. in a recent study, ruff and arbit (1981) describe a single case with 'aphemia' (apraxia of speech), whose ct scan reflects injury extending from broca's area to the inferior left precentral gyrus. these writers suggest that the articulatory disturbance which they refer to as 'aphemia' may have resulted from disruption of the connection between broca's area and the portion of the motor cortex that controls oral and pharyngeal muscles. mohr et al. (1978) have found that when a lesion lies outside broca's area and focally affects other adjacent sites in the operculum or insula, and even in a few instances apparently affects the deeper structures alone, the resulting syndromes are remarkably uniform, including dyspraxia and faint evidence of language disturbance. this provides some support for the findings in the present study, where a clearly identified lesion in broca's area was observed on the ct scan for f.t., but not for a.m. and e.j. the neuroanatomical findings in each case under study reflect lesions in the areas similar to those described in broca's original patient. broca, in describing the brain of his original aphasic patient, noted a large lesion encompassing the (l) insula, frontal, central and parietal operculum, and even extending into the adjacent inferior parietal region posterior to the sylvian fissure (mohr et al., 1978). he attributed this patient's aphasia to involvement of the frontal operculum, but not to the overall lesion. thus we see his readiness to emphasize only a small, focal portion of the overall lesion. mohr et al. (1978) feel that by modern standards, the initial physiologic basis for broca's clinico-pathological correlations seems dubious, and yet, the concept of an aphasia produced by infarction of the broca area has persisted. recent literature thus highlights the constraints produced by dogmatic adherence to the traditional formulation of the syndrome referred to as broca's aphasia, resulting from infarction of the broca's area. it is currently felt that the syndrome arises from a considerably larger brain injury in the sylvian region, that encompasses most of the operculum, insula and subjacent white matter in the territory of the middle cerebral artery serving the dominant cerebral hemisphere; thus far exceeding broca's area. this in fact conforms more to the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 lesley wolk large zone of damaged brain actually present in broca's original cases (roch-lecours and lhermitte, 1976). mohr et al. (1978) together with whitaker and seines (1975) contend that broca's aphasia emerges as a residuum of a larger infarct which initially produces global aphasia. they suggest that there is a gradual emergence of stereotypes, agrammatism and protracted apraxia of speech which evolve slowly towards the long-standing deficit profile of broca's aphasia. conclusion from the discussion above, it is concluded that there is a certain lawfulness to the apraxic impairment in these adult aphasic patients. articulation errors on single consonants and consonant clusters were characterized by systematic rule-governed behaviour. in each case, it was possible to trace underlying phonological processes which fit into strategy groups operationalizing particular phonetic preferences. the major process groups included a tendency for less complex syllable structures to replace more complex ones, a tendency towards phonetic symmetry and a tendency for errors to result from a lack of feature contrasts. in syllable structure processes, cluster reduction, deletion of final consonants and weak syllable deletion were most common. harmony processes were generally common, where assimilations occurred more frequently than metatheses and regressive assimilations outnumbered progressive assimilations. in feature contrast processes, fronting and stopping were clearly most common. similar error trends occurred on both singletons and clusters for all cases. analysis from a coarticulatory point of view indicated that some 'future scanning' appeared to be operating in these patients with apraxia of speech, resulting in assimilatory processes, feature merging and feature metathesis. it is thus argued that the impairment in these cases may not be confined to the motor aspects of speech, but probably extends into the linguistic spheres as well. this suggests that descriptions of apraxia of speech and clinical treatment regimes should perhaps consider both linguistic and articulatory variables. all cases employed a number of strategies to aid phonological production. this has several theoretical implications. in particular, revisualization of the written word and the use of silent articulations may indicate the preservation of relatively intact implicit speech patterns in these cases. all cases made several errors which were semantically related to the target word, yet fully explicable phonologically. this, combined with the fact that a carrier phrase with semantic relevance was often employed to aid phonological production, would suggest simultaneous semantic and phonological processing in these cases. this highlights direction for further research into the simultaneous processing or overlap between the levels of language in the aphasic patient. reduced speech rate was seen to facilitate improved phonological production, which may suggest a disruption in timing for these cases. the neuroanatomical findings suggest that apraxia of speech in these cases may result from some synergistic interaction between several areas of the brain rather than a specific localized area. this is in agreement with the view proposed by roch-lecours and lhermitte (1976) who suggest that particular language functions may be the result of an interaction of the entire opercular and insula regions. the findings indicate that apraxia of speech for these subjects does not appear to exist as a separate entity, but rather as a part of the total aphasic breakdown. parietal involvement suggests that enhanced kinaesthetic awareness and tactile discrimination of speech sounds should perhaps be more emphasized in therapy than has previously been considered. the fact that the brain lesion in each case was more extensive than would be expected in the traditional concept of a focal lesion in broca's area, would seem to suggest an orientation away from rigid diagnosic differentiation between broca and wernicke aphasic types. in this way, the speech pathologist might take cognisance of symptoms which could otherwise be overlooked. a phonological process analysis may form the basis for therapy by providing a way of describing the adult aphasic's phonological system as an entity, and allowing the clinician to isolate individual phonological processes contributing to unintelligibility. entire processes may thus be the focus of therapy rather than isolated sounds. clinically, it may be useful to explore the value of graphic symbols, covert articulatory movements and enhanced kinaesthetic awareness in the management of patients with apraxia of speech. in addition, treatment procedures involving rhythm, rate and syllable timing might be fruitful for future research. it is hoped that these findings will stimulate future investigations to improve both diagnostic and therapeutic efficacy and contribute to the endless search in understanding one of the most complex disorders of the human brain. acknowledgements the writer wishes to offer her sincere thanks to professor m. l. aron, head of the department of speech pathology and audiology and dr. c. penn, senior lecturer in the department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, for their guidance and support in carrying out this research. for financial assistance, the writer is grateful to the human sciences research council and the witwatersrand university senior bursary fund. ι references blumstein, s. e. a phonological investigation of aphasic speech. brown university and aphasia research centre, boston, the hague: mouton and co., 1973. | bowman, c. a. a process analysis of verbal apraxia. paper presented at the annual american speech-languagehearing association (asha) convention, toronto, canada, november, 1982. buckingham, h.w. explanation in apraxia with consequences for the concept of apraxia of speech. brain lang., 1979, 8, 202-226. buckingham, h. w. & rekart, d. m. semantic paraphasia.,/ commun. disord., 1979, 12(3), 197-209. cohen, j.α., gelfer, c.e. and sweet, r.d. neurologicneurosurgical notes: thalamic infarction producing aphasia. mt. sinai. j. med. (ny)., 47(4), 398-404^1980. critchley, m. articulatory defects in aphasia. j. laryngol. otol., 1952, 66, 1-17. deal, j. l. and darley, f. l. the influence of linguistic and situational variables on phonemic accuracy in apraxia of speech, j. speech hear. res., 1972, 15(3), 639-653. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) phonological and neuroanatomical findings in three cases with apraxia of speech 45 de renzi, e. and faglioni, p. normative data and screening power of a shortened version of the token test. cortex, 1978, 14(1), 41-49. di simoni, f.g. and darley, f.l. effect on phoneme duration control of three utterance-length conditions in an apractic patient. j. speech hear. dis., 1977,42,257-264. fromkin, v. a. the non-anomalous nature of anomalous utterances. language, 1971, 47, 27-52. goldman, r., fristoe, m. s. and woodcock, r. w. goldmanfristoe test of auditory discrimination. minnesota: american guidance service, inc., 1970. goodglass, h. and kaplan, e. the assessment of aphasia and related disorders. boston veterans administration hospital and aphasia research centre, department of neurology, boston university, 1972. halsey, j. η., blauenstein, u. w., wilson, ε. m. & wills, e. l. brain activation in the presence of brain damage. brain lang., 1980, 9(1), 47-60. ingram, d. phonological disability in children. studies in language rehabilitation and remediation 2. department of linguistics, university of british columbia: edward arnold, ltd., 1976. ingram, d. procedures for the phonological analysis of children's language. baltimore: university park press, 1981. itoh, m., sasanuma, s., hirose, h., yoshioka, h. andushijima, t. articulatory dynamics in a patient with apraxia of speech: x-ray microbeam observation. annu. bull. rilp., 1978, no. 12, 87-96. itoh, μ., sasanuma, s. and ushijima, t. velar movements during speech in a patient with apraxia of speech. brain lang., 1979, 7, 227-239. kimura, d. acquisition of a motor skill after left hemisphere damage. brain, 1977, 100, 527-542. klich, r. j., ireland, j. v. and weidner, w. e. articulatory and phonological aspects of consonant substitutions in apraxia of speech. cortex, 1979, 15,(3) 341-470. lebrun, y., buyssens, e. & henneaux, j. phonetic aspects ofanarthria. cortex, 1973,19, 126-135. / i martin, a. d. some objections to the term apraxia of speech'. j. speech hear. dis., 1974] 39(1), 53-64. martin, a. d. and rigrodsky, s. an investigation of phonological impairment in aphasia, part 1: cortex, 1974(a), 10(4) 317-328. i ι martin, a. d. and rigrodsky, s. an investigation of phonological impairment in aphasia, part 2: distinctive feature analysis of phonemic commutation errors in aphasia. cortex, 1974(b), 1064), 329-346. mcguigan, f. j. covert oral behaviour during the silent performance of language tasks. psychol. bull., 1970, 74, ' 309-326. mohr, j.p., pessin, m.s., finkelstein, s., funkenstein, η.η., duncan, g. w. and davis, k. r. broca aphasia: pathologic and clinical. neurology, 1978, 28, 311-324. moore, w.m., rosenbek, j.c. and la pointe, l. l. ayiessment of oral apraxia in brain-injured adults. proceedings of clinical aphasiology conference, portland, oregon, may, 1976. nathan, p. w. facial apraxia and apraxic dysarthria. brain, 19487, 70, 449-478. nebes, r. d. the nature of internal speech in a patient with aphemia. brain. lang., 1975, 2, 489-497. peuser, g. and fittschen, m. on the universality of language dissolution: the case of a turkish aphasic. brain, lang., 1977, 4, 196-207. roch-lecours, h. and lhermitte, f. the pure form of the phonetic disintegration syndrome (pure anarthria). brain. lang., 1976, 3, 88-113. ruff, r. l. and arbit, e. aphemia resulting from a left frontal hematoma. neurology, 1981, 31, 353-356. sarno, μ. t. and levita, e. natural course of recovery in severe aphasia. arch. phys. rehabil., 1971, 52, 175-178. sasanuma, s. speech characteristics of a patient with apraxia of speech. section of communication research, department of rehabilitation medicine. tokyo metropolitan institute of gerontology, tokyo, japan. 1971. warren, r.l. rehearsal of naming in 'apraxia of speech'. proceedings of clinical aphasiology conference, 1977. 80-90. weiner, f. f. phonological process analysis. baltimore: university park press, 1979. whitaker, h. a. and seines, o. a. broca's area: a problem in language-brain relationships. linguistics an international review, 154/155, june 15, 1975, 91-101. wolk, l. phonological impairment in aphasia. paper presented at the annual american speech-language-hearing association (asha) convention, toronto, canada, november, 1982(a). wolk, l. phonological impairment in aphasia. unpublished dissertation submitted to the department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, south africa, 1982(b). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 31, 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gsi 28 auto tymp the gsi 28 auto tymp provides testing capability for tympanometry, ipsilateral and contralateral acoustic reflex testing and screening audiometry. selection of test sequence is as simple as pressing a button! the" auto tymp is lightweight and compact so it can be easily moved from one location to another. an optional carrying case is available if more portability is required. the needier westdene organisation (pty) limited in association with / in medewerking met hearing and acoustic instruments (pty) ltd lewis's hearing centre (pty) ltd engineered acoustic products noise control needier westdene house, 33 durham st., raedene, p.o. box 28975, sandringham 2131, south africa. johannesburg 2192, south africa. telex: 4-25028. tel: (011) 640-5017. cables: needlerog. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 31 1984 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 23 'η ondersoek na die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van premature hoerisikokinders helene j.e. smit, b.log (pretoria) brenda louw, d.phil (pretoria) isabel c. uys, d.phil (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria. opsomming . die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van ses afrikaanssprekende hoerisikokinders tussen die ouderdom dertig tot sewe-en-dertig maande is geevalueer aan die hand van die drie taaldimensies, naamlik taalvorm, taalinhoud en taalgebruik, aangesien kliniese bewyse daarop dui dat vertraagde taalontwikkeling meer algemeen onder premature hoerisikokinders voorkom. 'n beeld van die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van die kinders is verkry, en daar is gevind dat die proefpersone 'n beperkte ekspressiewe woordeskat sowel as 'n ontwikkelingsagterstand van ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens vertoon. die belang van die rol van die spraakterapeut m die vroee identifisering, evaluasie, behandeling en opvolg van die kommunikasievermoens van premature jong kinders, word deur die bevmdings van hierdie studie beklemtoon. abstract the expressive communicative ability of six afrikaans speaking high-risk children between the ages of thirty to thirty seven months was evaluated on the basis of the three dimensions of language form, language content, and language use, as clinical evidence points to the fact that retarded language development more often occurs amongst premature high-risk children. a picture of the communicative ability of the children was obtained and it was found that the subjects tested manifested a limited expressive vocabulary as well as a developmental retardation of expressive communicative ability. the importance of the role of the speech therapist with regard to the early identification, evaluation, treatment and follow-up of the communicative ability of premature young children is emphasised by the findings of this study. vroee diensleweringsprogramme vir jong hoerisikokinders word tans deur verskeie ontwikkelingspesialisasiegebiede gepropageer in 'n poging om die invloed van hoerisikofaktore op die kind se ontwikk'eling te modifiseer of selfs te oorkom (liebergott, bashir enlschultz, 1984). belangstelling in die unieke behoeftes van hoerisikokinders het in die afgelope twee dekades toegeneem, en het gelei tot die ontwikkeling van die relatief nuwe vakgebied van vroee intervensie by hoerisikokinders deur 'n multi-dissiplinere span. die hoerisikobaba is 'n baba by'wie daar as gevolg van verskeie faktore wat tydens die prenatale, perinatale en neonatale geskiedenis voorkom, 'n groter as normale risiko bestaan dat hy 'n ontwikkelingsagterstand en later potensiele ontwikkelingsprobleme sal vertoon. geboortegewig en gestasie-ouderdom word as die primere bepalers van hoerisikostatus beskou (rosetti, 1986). prematuriteit word deur liebergott et al. (1984) omskryf as 'n biologiese risikofaktor wat inmeng met die ontwikkelende sentrale senuweestelsel wat die moontlikheid van latere afwykende ontwikkeling verhoog. usher (in rossetti, 1986) kategoriseer prematuriteit op grond van die graad daarvan en hy onderskei tussen drie kategoriee, naamlik grensprematuriteit (37-38 weke); matige prematuriteit (31-36 weke) en ekstreme prematuriteit (24-30 weke). kliniese bewyse dui daarop dat vertraagde taalontwikkeling meer algemeen onder premature hoerisikobabas voorkom, maar daar is min empiriese bewyse ter stawing van hierdie verskynsel (hubatch, johnson, kristler, burns en moneka, 1985). die belang van deeglike ondersoek van die taalen kommunikasieontwikkeling van jong hoerisikokinders, met die oog op die vroee identifisering en behandeling blyk duidelik uit die literatuur (marge, 1984; louw, 1986). aram en nation (1980) en strominger en bashir (1977) is van mening dat vroee versteurings van taalen spraakontwikkeling die voorlopers van latere leerprobleme is. vertraagde en afwykende spraak-, taalen gehoorontwikkeling word dikwels eers ge'identifiseer wanneer die kind skoolgaande ouderdom bereik. op daardie stadium het die kritiese taalaanleerperiode reeds verstryk, en taalopleidingsprogramme kompenseer nie altyd voldoende vir die verlies aan tyd tydens die normale ontwikkelingsperiode nie (ehrlich, shapiro, kimball en huttner, 1973; cole, 1982). afwykende taalvermoens kan ook 'n nadelige invloed uitoefen op die kind se interpersoonlike groei, verwerwing van kennis en vaslegging van 'n eie identiteit (cole, 1982). dit blyk dus dat die voorkoms van hoerisikotoestande in die kind se ontwikkelingsgeskiedenis daarop dui dat die kind onder 'n groter risiko vir die ontwikkeling van kommunikasieprobleme verkeer (liebergott et al. 1984). liebergott et al. (1986) is egter van mening dat slegs longitudinale studies van jong hoerisikokinders se taalontwikkeling, die vroee identifisering van die jong hoerisikokinders wat latere taalprobleme kan ontwikkel moontlik sal maak. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 © sasha 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 van wee die huidige gebrekkige navorsing oor die kommunikasievermoens van hoerisikokinders, en die negatiewe gevolge wat vroee kommunikasieafwykings op latere ontwikkeling kan he, word die volgende vrae gestel: hoe vertoon vroee kommunikasieontwikkeling van premature hoerisikokinders en is hulle kommunikasieontwikkeling van so aard dat dit later ontwikkeling wel kan benadeel? 'n algemene kenmerk van prematuur gebore jong kinders is 'n vertraagde taalontwikkeling (kastein en fowler, 1959 en field, dempsey en shuman, 1979). 'n beduidende vertraging in die ekspressiewe en reseptiewe taalvermoens van premature jong kinders is deur verskeie navorsers gei'dentifiseer (hubatch et al. 1985, en wright, thistlewaite, elton, wilkinson en forfor, 1983). hierdie kinders vertoon ook dikwels 'n beperkte aantal uitinge as gevolg van 'n beperkte ekspressiewe woordeskat, asook woordvindingsprobleme, probleme met sinsuitbreiding, die verskaffing van definisies van begrippe, 'n vertraging in die produksie van sinne en die weergee van gegewe getikte ritmepatrone (hubatch et al. 1985 en de hirsch, jansky en langford, 1964). volgens hubatch et al. (1981) is hierdie kinders ook onderhewig aan die ontwikkeling van latere kommunikasieprobleme. 'n hoe voorkoms van leesprobleme is onder prematuur gebore kinders gei'dentifiseer, wat nie net in geskrewe taal nie, maar ook in gesproke taal manifesteer (douglas, 1960; pasamanick en knobloch, 1960; benton, 1962). vertraagde spraakontwikkeling, wat dikwels nog tot op skoolgaande ouderdom voorkom, kom ook algemeen onder hoerisikokinders voor (fitzhardinge, 1980). jong hoerisikokinders presenteer dikwels met konduktiewe gehoorverliese, wat ook tot vertraagde spraaken taalontwikkeling kan lei (northern en downs, 1984; hubatch et al. 1985). aangesien vroee kommunikasieprobleme in 'n latere globale kommunikasievertraging kan presenteer, is die vroee identifikasie en gereelde opvolg van hierdie kinders van kardinale belang (hubatch et al. 1985). die toepassingswaarde van bogenoemde studies in terme van die vroee identifisering en die behandeling van hoerisikokinders is egter beperk as gevolg van metodologiese verskille tussen die studies (liebergott et al. 1984). ten spyte van die beperkte navorsing en die uiteenlopende aard van die beskikbare navorsing oor die kommunikasieontwikkeling van jong hoerisikokinders, dui bevindinge op die voorkoms van vroee kommunikasieprobleme en die belang van vroee ingryping om hierdie probleme te identifiseer en op te hef (marge, 1984; louw, 1986). daar bestaan egter 'n gebrek aan skakeling tussen eksperimentele navorsing en die kliniese praktyk. slegs enkele hoerisikokinders word in die kliniese praktyk na die spraakterapeut verwys vir ondersoek en behandeling. op grond van die belang van vroee ingryping wat in die literatuur uitgewys is, is die aandag wat aan jong hoerisikokinders in die praktyk geskenk word dus onvoldoende. vanwee die dringende behoefte wat bestaan aan verdere empiriese navorsing oor jong hoerisikokinders se kommunikasieontwikkeling, is die doel van hierdie studie die bestudering van die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van afrikaanssprekende hoerisikokinders. metodologie doelstellings die hoofdoel van hierdie studie is om die ekspressiewe komh e l e n e j . . smit, brenda louw en isabel c. uys munikasieontwikkelingsvlak van 'n groep premature hoerisikokinders te ondersoek. bogenoemde doelstelling word bereik deur die evaluering en omskrywing van die proefpersone se taalinhoud, taalvorm en taalgebruik. proefpersoonseleksie die volgende kriteria van seleksie van proefpersone is gestel, naamlik: — prematuriteit al die proefpersone moes drie of meer weke prematuur gebore wees, dit wil se as grenslyn premature gevalle gediagnoseer wees. prematuriteit word as 'n biologiese risiko vir die ontwikkeling van kommunikasievermoens beskou (liebergott et al. 1984; rosetti, 1986). — ouderdom alle proefpersone moes tussen die ouderdom van 30 tot 36 maande wees aangesien die doel van die studie die bepaling is van die vroee ekspressiewe kommunikasieontwikkelingsvlak by jong kinders wat prematuur gebore is. — huistaal die kinders moes almal afrikaanssprekend wees, aangesien die doel van die studie die bestudering van die ekspressiewe kommunikasieontwikkelingsvlak van afrikaanssprekende hoerisikokinders is. — geassosieerde afwykings die proefpersone moes geen geassosieerde afwykings wat noodwendige kommunikasieprobleme tot gevolg het, byvoorbeeld breinskade, vertoon nie. (sien tabel 1.) voorlopig is 30 proefpersone uit die geboorte-rekords van die kraamafdeling van 'n provinsiale hospitaal, asook die geboorte-rekords van twee pediaters in privaat praktyke, deur middel van toevallige steekproeftrekking geselekteer. vanuit die 30 voorlopige proefpersone wat geselekteer is, jis ses kinders as proefpersone vir die studie geneem. 1 apparaat | i speelgoed is op grond van die kinders se belangstelling geselekteer vir ontlokking van 'n spontane taalmonster. 'n bandopnemer (sanyo model μ 4200) en oudio-kasette (emi sp 90) is benut vir die beoordeling van die proefpersone se ekspressiewe kommunikasikevermoens. die proefpersone se middeloorfunksionering is met behulp van 'n outomatiese siftingsimpedansmeter (tympanometer model 85 ar) bepaal. / materiaal die proefpersone se ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens is geevalueer volgens hulle taalinhoud, taalvorm en taalgebruik (bloom en lahey, 1978) en word vervolgens in tabelvorm verskaf, na aanleiding van die drie dimensies van taal wat geevalueer is. (sien tabel 2.) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η ondersoek na die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van premature hoerisikokinders tabel 1: beskrywing van die proefpersone 25 kenmerke geslag van kind ouderdom van kind massa van kind by geboorte aantal weke prematuur probleme met gesondheid waar kind gedurende dag bly aantal kinders in gesin: seuns dogters hoeveelste kind in gesin is proefpersoon probleme met swangerskap en behandeling tipe geboorte posisie van kind by geboorte hulpmiddels gebruik by geboorte toestand van kind na geboorte beroep van ouers: moeder vader hoogste standerd geslaag: moeder vader naskoolse kwalifikasies: moeder vader ouderdom van: moeder [ vader i bj md jf jl cw rb vroulik vroulik vroulik manlik manlik manlik ': -naande 35 maande 34 maande 33 maande 30 maande 37 maande 1,5 kg 1,99 kg 2,73 kg 3,18 kg 2,9 kg 2,19 kg 7 weke 6 weke 3 weke 7 weke 3,5 weke 6 weke geen geen diken duru geen geen gereelde mangelderm probleme ontsteking en en blindederm swak tande is verwyder op 6e dag moeder moeder moeder ouma moeder & 2 dae by bediende en moeder by vriendin twee oggende in speelgroepie 0 0 1 1 2 1 1 1 1 0 1 1 eerste eerste tweede eerste derde eerste kalmeermiddels geen inspuiting, drup verwydering dreigende misplasentale vir emosionele en hospitalisasie van nierstene kraam afwyking skok op 7 mnde vir epilepsie op (dood van suster) 7 maande natuurlik natuurlik natuurlik natuurlik natuurlik keisersnee kop eerste kop eerste kop eerste kop eerste kop eerste kop eerste instrumente/ geen geen geen medikasie vir geen induksie moeder in broeikas kalsiumoortapblindedermopein broeikas geelsug. drup ping. in broeikas. rasie op 6e dag. vir 1 week. in geelsug bloedoortapping broeikas. op 8e dag. huisvrou huisvrou huisvrou kredietklerk maatskaplike asst. rekenmeeswerker ter landdros admin. bestuurambagsman — wetenskaplike regs adm. beder ampte st 10 st 10 st 9 st 8 st 10 st 10 st 10 st 10 st8 — st 10 st 10 radiografiesekretariele geen geen ba maatskapb. juris diploma diploma like werk b. juris llb b. juris ba-graad ambag (passer/ — bsc rek.wetenb. juris llb b. juris draaier) skap. 35 jr 25 jr 25 jr 22 jr 30 jr 26 jr 34 jr 28 jr 30 jr — 32 jr 28 jr tabel 2: opsomming van evaluasiewyses vir ondersoek van ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens i taaldimensie wat ondersoek is evaluasiewyses vir ondersoek van ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens taalinhoud dore (1979) se metode vir die analise van gesprekskategoriee. taalvorm atrikaanse artikulasie-ondersoek (lotter, 1979) eerste ondersoek van die orofasiale meganisme (louw en van der merwe, 1981) afrikaanse vorm van ''language assessment, remediation and screening procedure" (crystal, fletcher & garman, 1976) taalgebruik moeder-kind-kommunikasie-interaksie-skaal (clezy, 1979) bates (1977) se metode vir die analise van (die kategoriee van kommunikatiewe gebare. eksperimentele prosedure — naturalistiese observasie is in die tuissituasie gemaak, om sodoende so 'n natuurlik moontlike omgewing te skep, sodat die proefpersone op hul gemak is vir die verkryging van 'n verteenwoordigende spraakmonster (bloom en lahey, 1978; lund en duchan, 1983). — proefpersone is in 'n spelsituasie waartydens natuurlike wyse van kommunikasie ontlok is, waargeneem. 'n dertigminuut-bandopname asook aantekeninge is deur die ondersoeker gemaak oor die aktiwiteite van die kind en moeder. — eers het die moeder van die proefpersoon, en daarna die ondersoeker met die kind vir 'n periode van 15 minute elk, tydens spel, in interaksie getree. daar is besluit op 'n interaksieperiode van 30 minute om uitputting van die proefpersoon te voorkom en aangesien die tyd genoegsaam is om verskeie analiseringsprosedures te implementeer (miller, 1981). — spel het tydens die interaksie plaasgevind in 'n vertrek die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 helene j.ε. smit, brenda louw en isabel c. uys deur die ouers geselekteer, om sodoende die toetssituasie minder formeel te maak. — kontak is eers deur die ondersoeker met die kind opgebou alvorens met die toetsing begin is. — die volgende instruksies is aan die moeder verskaf sodat sy geweet het wat van haar verlang word tydens toetsing: — sy moet op so 'n natuurlik moontlike wyse met die kind tydens spel in interaksie optree soos wat sy gewoonlik doen. — 'n vertrek van haar keuse moet geselekteer word om 'n natuurlike omgewing te verseker. — spel moet op die vloer plaasvind. — die kind moet toegelaat word om self te kies waarmee hy wil speel, om sodoende die ontlokking van spraak te verseker. — die duur van die kommunikasie-interaksie moet 15 minute wees. die ondersoeker sal die moeder in ken* nis stel wanneer die ondersoeker by haar oorneem. — 'n bandopname, asook aantekeninge sal deur die ondersoeker gemaak word, om sodoende die optekening van resultate te vergemaklik. — die afrikaanse artikulasie-ondersoek (lotter, 1979) is na spelinteraksie met elke proefpersoon uitgevoer. — die orofasiale ondersoek van elke kind is laastens uitgevoer aangesien beter samewerking so verkry kan word. optekening en analise van data die optekening en analise van data is vergemaklik deur middel van die gebruik van 'n bandopname, asook aantekeninge oor aktiwiteite en beskrywing van die nie-verbale konteks, gedurende die kommunikasiesituasie (miller, 1981). die optekening van data geskied aan die hand van riglyne wat in die literatuur verskaf word, en word opsommend in tabel 3 weergegee. in die tabelle word die prestasies van die verskillende proefpersone, vir die verskillende toetse, individueel en indien toepaslik, gesamentlik aangetoon. vir vergelykingsdoeleindes is daar van persentasies en rekeningkundige gemiddeldes gebruik gemaak (ferguson, 1981). die wyse waarop die data opgeteken en geanaliseer is, word in tabelvorm beskryf. (sien tabel 3.) resultate en bespreking dit blyk dat die proefpersone as groep 'n ontwikkelingsagterstand vertoon wat hulle ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens aanbetref, aangesien hulle ekspressiewe kommunikasievaardighede ooreenstem met die van jonger kinders (bloom en lahey, 1978). die resultate word aan die hand van die evaluasiekategoriee van taalinhoud, taalvorm en taalgebruik bespreek. taalinhoud die taalinhoud van die proefpersone is geevalueer deur die toepassing van dore (1978) se metode vir die analise van gesprekskategoriee. die resultate van die ses proefpersone se taalinhoud word in tabel 4 verskaf. die gesprekskategoriee is in tabel 4 gerangskik in volgorde van afname van die gemiddelde persentasie van voorkoms van die proefpersone. die voorkoms van gesprekskategoriee vir elke proefpersoon is ook in terme van persentasies uitgedruk. uit die resultate blyk dit dat die kategorie van response die meeste voorgekom het, naamlik met 'n gemiddelde persentasie van 63,2% terwyl die kategorie van performatiewe totaal afwesig was. die hoe voorkoms van die responsekategorie kan verklaar word aan die hand van rodgon (1979) se bevindings dat kinders tussen een en 'n half tot twee jaar, reeds kan diskrimineer tussen "ja/nee-vrae" en "w-vrae" — vrae met vreemde voornaamwoorde, soos byvoorbeeld wat, wie, waar — en toepaslik op die vrae kan antwoord. al die proefpersone het toepaslik op alle "ja/neevrae" en "w-vrae" geantwoord. die kategorie van organisasie met 'n gemiddelde voorkomsfrekwensie van 11,4% het die tweede hoogste voorkoms getoon en die resultate stem ooreen met bevindings van bloom et al. (1976) en keenan (1974), wat van mening is dat organisasiemiddele reeds in die dialoog van tweetot driejarige kinders behoort voor te kom. die resultate van die proefpersone stem dus ooreen met die beskrywing van normale ontwikkeling van engelssprekende kinders, wat in die literatuur verskaf word. die kategorie van versoeke het die derde hoogste voorkomsfrekwensie vertoon (10,1%) wat korreleer met resultate van studies van die normale ontwikkeling van engelssprekende kinders van tyack et al. (1979) en wood (1981). hulle het bevind dat kinders vanaf tweetot vierjarige ouderdom "ja/nee-vrae" en "w-vrae" gebruik. versoeke het minder voorgekom as response, waarskynlik omdat response op "ja/nee-vrae" en "w-vrae" reeds begin ontwikkel voordat normaal ontwikkelende kinders die vrae kan vra (rodgon, 1979). slegs 8,8% van die kategoriebeskrywings het voorgekom, moontlik aangesien voorskoolse kinders dikwels hiermee probleme ondervind, en die ontwikkeling van beskrywings tot in die skooljare voortduur (wood, 1981). slegs 4,4% van die kategorie van erkennings het voorgekom. kinders van tweetot driejarige ouderdom verwag reeds erkennings op hulle uitinge en gebruik erkennings om aan die spreker te kenne te gee dat hulle die informasie wat hy wil oordra, ontvang het — moontlik om die vloei van kommunikasie aan die gang te hou (rodgon, 1979; cole, 1982). die' lae voorkoms van die gebruik van stellings, naamlik (2,1%) korreleer met opvattings van bloom en lahey (1978) dat normale tweetot driejariges nie altyd oor hulle gevoelens, houdings en oortuigings praat nie, moontlik omdat hulle nie weet hoe om daaroor te praat nie. attribusies word egter reeds vanaf een jaar tien maande deur engelssprekende kinders gebruik (ainsfeld, 1984). j uit bogenoemde bespreking skyn die proefpersone se taalinhoud ooreen te stem met die literatuurbeskrywing van normale engelssprekende kinders se ontwikkeling van taalinhoud, behalwe vir die gesprekskategorie van performatiewe wat vertraagde ontwikkeling vertoon, moontlik omdat die moeders tydens waarneming glad nie van die gesprekskategorie in hulle interaksie met die proefpersone gebruik gemaak het nie. moontlik beskik die proefpersone oor die gebruik daarvan, maar het dit nie tydens die taalopname spontaan gebruik nie. 'n voorstel kan egter gemaak word dat 'n ondersoeker sodanige kategoriee wat nie spontaan voorkom nie, moet ontlok (miller, 1981), taalvorm die evaluasie van taalvorm het die evaluasie van die artikulasievermoens, orofasiale meganisme en ekspressiewe taalthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) η ondersoek na die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van premature hoerisikokinders tabel 3: optekening en analise van data taaldimensie evaluasiewyses optekening van data analise van data taalinhoud dore (1978) se metode vir die analise van gesprekskategoriee transkripsie van bandopname van interaksie tussen moeder en kind, en ondersoeker en kind. analise van data aan die hand van verskeie gesprekskategoriee: versoeke beskrywings erkennings performatiewe-response stellings organisasie ander, in terme van aantal kere van voorkoms, toepaslikheid en ontoepaslike gebruik (dore, 1978). taalvorm afrikaanse artikulasie ondersoek (lotter, 1979) optekening van data op toepaslike vorm. analise van data aan die hand van vermoe en onvermoe tot produksie van enkelklanke en konsonantkombinasies in inisiele, mediale en finale woordposisies in terme van: klankomruilings klankweglatings klankdistorsies klankvervangings (van riper) eerste ondersoek van die orofasiale meganisme (louw & v.d. merwe, 1981) optekening van data op die voorgeskrewe vorm. analise van data op grond van normale of afwykende vorm in funksie van al die spraakorgane, soos voorgeskrewe vorm bepaal (louw en v.d. merwe, 1981). afrikaanse vorm van "language assessment, remediation and screening procedure" (crystal, fletcher en garman, 1976) transkripsie van bandopname van die interaksie tussen die moeder en kind, en die ondersoeker en kind se optekening van resultate van analise op die vertaalde larsp-profiel, soos voorgeskryf analise van data vind plaas aan die hand van 4 vlakke van strukturele organisasie, naamlik: frasevlak sinsvlak woordtipes bysinne (crystal et al. 1976) taalgebruik / / moeder-k teraksie (( ind-kommunikasie-in:iyzy, 1979) transkripsie van 15-minuutbandopname van interaksie tussen moeder en kind en optekening van resultate op vertaalde vorm van die versterkingsprofiel. analise van data aan die hand van 5 kategoriee deur clezy (1978) bepaal: positiewe kommentaar van moeder. korrekte leer van kind. onderrig van moeder aan kind. foutiewe leer van kind. negatiewe kommentaar van moeder. die totaal van elke kategorie word verdeel deur die tydsduur van die spraakmonster om die snelheid per minuut te verkry. bates (19 analise v. kommun: 77) se metode vir die in die kategoriee van katiewe gebare. / data word opgeteken op opgestelde vorm, op grond van aantekeninge gemaak van die nie-verbale kommunikasiekonteks tydens die interaksie. data word geanaliseer op grond van die aanwesigheid of afwesigheid asook die toepaslikheid of ontoepaslikheid van die volgende kategoriee van kommunikatiewe gebare, nl: wys na voorwerpe vertoon van voorwerpe gee voorwerpe aan 'n volwassene uitreik weiering afwys ( bates, 1977) vermoens van die proefpersone gedek en word volgens hierdie indeling bespreek. artikulasievermoens die foutklanke en frekwensie foute van elke proefpersoon, sowel as die persentasie proefpersone wat elke foutklank foutief geproduseer het, soos verkry deur die toepassing van die afrikaanse artikulasie-ondersoek (lotter, 1974), is bereken (smit, 1986). verskeie artikulasiefoute met onderlinge variasie het tussen die proefpersone voorgekom. die artikulasiefoute het slegs uit klankvervangings en klankweglatings bestaan, waarvan klankweglatings die meeste voorgekom het. klankkombinasies is die meeste aangetas, veral die produksie van /r/die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34,1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 h e l e n e j . . smit, brenda louw en isabel c. uys tabel 4: opsommende tabel van dore (1978) se metode vir die analise van gesprekskategoriee kategorie % voorkoms van kategoriee per proefpersoon kategorie bj md jf jl cw rb gemid. response 59,3 67,0 65,2 66,3 61,8 59,5 63,2 organisasie 12,0 10,0 10,6 8,1 16,0 11,8 11,4 versoeke 17,6 7,3 12,1 9,3 6,2 8,1 10,1 beskrywings 8,3 8,3 5,0 11,6 13,6 5,9 8,8 erkennings 0,0 2,8 5,0 3,5 1,2 14,0 4,4 stellings 2,8 4,6 2,1 1,2 1,2 0,7 2,1 performatiewe 0,0 0,0 0,0 0,0 0,0 0,0 0,0 totaal 100,0 100,0 100,0 100,0 100,0 100,0 100,0 kombinasies in die aanvangsso wel as die eindposisie. hierdie resultate stem ooreen met die van lotter (1974), wat bevind dat afrikaanssprekende kinders eers vanaf 9,25 tot 9,45 jaar 100% korrekte produksie van klankkombinasies van /r/, korrek kan produseer. foutiewe produksie van /j/ het by 100% van die proefpersone voorgekom. afrikaanssprekende kinders bemeester egter eers 100% korrekte produksie van /j/ op ongeveer 7,5 tot 8,42 jaar (lotter, 1974). volgens die norme van spraakontwikkeling van afrikaanse kinders, blyk hierdie proefpersone se spraakontwikkeling egter binne normale perke vir hulle chronologiese ouderdom te wees (lotter, 1974) en hulle artikulasiefoute kan as normale ontwikkelingsfoute beskou word (weiss, lillywhite en gordon, 1980). aangesien fonetiese vermoens be'invloed word deur die ryping van die artikulators, mag die normale ontwikkelingsfoute wat die proefpersone vertoon, moontlik toegeskryf word aan die feit dat die artikulators van die jong kinders eers met toename in ouderdom tot voile ryping sal kom (weiss et al. 1980). eerste ondersoek van die orofasiale meganisme met die ondersoek van die orofasiale meganisme van die proefpersone is daar geen afwykings in die vorm of funksie van die spraakorgane, soos deur die voorgeskrewe vorm bepaal, ge'identifiseer nie. die vorm en funksie van die spraakorgane behoort dus nie hulle artikulasievermoens negatief te be'invloed nie (weiss et al. 1980) en daar is dan ook bevind dat die proefpersone wel net artikulatoriese ontwikkelingsfoute vertoon het. die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van die proefpersone in tabel 5 word die resultate van elke proefpersoon soos tabel 5: opsommende weergawe van die proefpersone se ekspressiewe taalvorm na aanleiding van die larsp-prosedure verkry met die toepassing van die afrikaanse weergawe van die larsp-tegniek (crystal, fletcher en garman, 1976) opsommend weergegee. in tabel 5 word die totale frekwensie van voorkoms van sinen frasestrukture van elke proefpersoon op stadia een tot vyf van sintaktiese ontwikkeling weergegee. die stadium van sintaktiese ontwikkeling waarop elke proefpersoon gefunksioneer het, word beskou as die stadium waar elke proefpersoon die grootste totale frekwensie sinstrukture en frasestrukture vertoon het. uit die resultate blyk dat al die proefpersone wat sintaktiese ontwikkeling betref, ongeveer ses maande onder hulle toepaslike oudersomsvlakke gefunksioneer het (pieterse, 1969 en crystal et al. 1976). vyftig persent van die proefpersone het 'n groot aantal onverstaanbare sinne getoon. daar was 'n hoe voorkoms van die gebruik van voornaamwoorde en koppelwerkwoorde onder vier van die proefpersone naamlik bj, md en rb, wat op stadium 3 van sintaktiese ontwikkeling funksioneer. die bevindings stem ooreen met resultate deur pieterse (1969) verkry ten opsigte van normaalontwikkelende afrikaanse kinders wat op stadium 3 gefunksioneer het. verkleinwoorde is geredelik deur alle proefpersone gebruik. dit kan toegeskryf word aan die hoe voorkoms daarvan soos waargeneem is in die moeders se kommunikasie met die kinders (clezy, 1979; cole, 1982). 'n groot aantal herhalings het ook in die sintaktiese strukture van die proefpersone voorgekom, wat ooreenstem met resultate van hubatch et al. (1985), wat die verskynsel toeskryf aan 'n beperkte ekspressiewe woordeskat van die kinders. die gemiddelde sinlengtes van die proefpersone bestaan uit 1,67 tot 2,76 woorde, maar volgens die ontwikkelingsnorme behoort hulle egter reeds drietot vierwoordsinne te kan gebruik (pieterse, 1969; crystal et al. 1976). 'n beperkte aantal uitinge asook probleme met die uitbreiding van sinne kom by die proefpersone voor, soos ook bevind is in navorsing deur hubatch et al. (1985) en de hirsch et al. (1984) uitgevoer. (sien tabel 6.) tabel 6: opsommende weergawe van die sinsproduksie van die proefpersone na aanleiding van die larsp-prosedure veranderlike proefpersoon 1 veranderlike bj md jf jl cw rb totale aantal woorde 200 237 212 143 127 239 totale aantal sinne 87 86 95 73 76 96 gemiddelde sinlengte 2,30 2,76 2,23 1,96 1,67 2,49 onvolledige sinne 10 5 1 0 1 2 onverstaanbare sinne 0 1 24 28 48 2 'n aantal onvolledige sinne het,ook voorgekom, moontlik as gevolg van probleme wat die kinders ondervind het; om verskillende eenhede in 'n enkele sin te kombineer (ainsfield, 1984), en wanneer die kinders se aandag na 'n nuwe fokus verskuif het. die kinders se spraakverstaanbaarheid is hierdeur negatief be'invloed. uit bogenoemde bespreking wil dit dus voorkom of die proefpersone as 'n groep 'n beperkte ekspressiewe woordeskat vertoon het en 'n ekspressiewe ontwikkelingsagterstand van ongeveer ses maande onder hulle chronologiese ouderdomsvlak vertoon het. die resul•kategorie proefpersone en frekwensie sin& frasestrukture •kategorie bj md jf jl cw rb 1. 9-18 maande 37 32 34 32 37 10 2. 18-24 maande 45 59 73 51 62 75 3. 24-30 maande 117 101 127 28 24 164 4. 30-36 maande 3 23 20 16 0 34 5. 36-42 maande 0 3 3 0 0 1 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) -η ondersoek na die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van premature hoerisikokinders 29 tate stem ooreen met die resultate van premature kinders wat reeds in die literatuur bestudeer is (de hirsch et al. 1964). taalgebruik die evaluasie van taalgebruik het die aspekte van moederkind-kommunikasie-interaksie en kommunikatiewe gebare deur die proefpersone gebruik, gedek en word in die volgorde bespreek. die moeder-kind-kommunikasie-interaksie tabel 7 verskaf 'n visuele voorstelling van die resultate van die moeder-kind-kommunikasie-interaksie. die voorkoms van die vyf kategoriee van moeder-kind-kommunikasieinteraksie volgens clezy (1979), naamlik positiewe kommentaar van die moeder, negatiewe kommentaar van die moeder, korrekte en foutiewe leer van die kind en die onderrig van die kind deur die moeder, word in tabel 7 as 'n persentasie van die totale interaksies tussen elke moeder en kind weergegee. tabel 7: resultate van die moeder-kind-kommunisie-interaksie kategoriee volgens clezy (1979) proefpersoon kategoriee volgens clezy (1979) bj md jf jl cw rb positiewe kommentaar van moeder 9,1 9,3 5,0 5,0 7,4 10,4 kind leer korrek 30,3 29,1 38,3 27,5 26,5 31,2 moeder onderrig 42,8 kind 50,0 47,4 46,7 37,3 42, b 42,8 kind leer foutief 9,1 10,6 8,3 13,9 13,2 7,8 negatiewe kommentaar van moeder 1,5 3,6 1,7 16,3 10,3 7,8 totaal (persentasie) 100,0 100,0 100,0 100,0 100,0 100,0 oor die algemeen het daar meer positiewe kommentaar van die moeders (7,7%) as negatiewe kommentaar (6,8%) voorgekom. proefpersone cw en jl het egter meer negatiewe as positiewe kommentaar van hulle moeders ontvang, en het ook die minste korrekte leer en die meeste foutiewe leer vertoon in verglyking met die persentasie vir hierdie kategoriee by die ander proefpersone. hierdie verskynsel kan moontlik toegeskryf word aan 'n wedersydse negatiewe versterking tussen moeder en kind (clezy, 1979). die moeders van die proefpersone, as 'n groep het heelwat uitbreidings, herhalings, retoriese vrae en selfbeantwoording van eie vrae as taalonderrigmiddele gebruik. hierdie waarneming stem ooreen met literatuurbevindings ten opsigte van moeders met normaal ontwikkelende kinders (bloom en lahey, 1978). bogenoemde gebruik van taalonderrigmiddele deur die moeders mag ook aanleiding gegee het tot die hoer persentasie korrekte leer van die proefpersone, naamlik van 26,5 tot 38,3% teenoor die laer persentasie van foutiewe leer deur die proefpersone vertoon, naamlik van 7,8 tot 13,9% (clezy, 1979). die feit dat cw en jl se sintaktiese vermoens en spraakverstaanbaarheid ook die swakste van al die proefpersone was, mag ook moontlik toegeskryf word aan die beperkende invloed wat te veel negatiewe kommentaar van 'n moeder op 'n kind se spraak mag he (clezy, 1979). oor die algemeen proefpersone cw en jl uitgesonder blyk dit dat die moederkind-kommunikasie-interaksie van die proefpersone oorwegend 'n positiewe, fassiliterende invloed op die kinders se taalontwikkeling uitgeoefen het, moontlik omdat die ouers besorgd was oor hulle kinders se prematuriteit en daarom gepoog het om goeie modelle te wees. kommunikatiewe gebare deur die proefpersone gebruik tabel 8 verskaf die resultate van elke proefpersoon in terme van die teenwoordigheid en toepaslike gebruik van die kategoriee van kommunikatiewe gebare volgens bates (1977). uit tabel 8: analise van die kategoriee van kommunikatiewe gebare (bates, 1977) proefpersoon kategorie 1. wys na voorwerpe:| (a) nie-kommunikatief (b) kommunikatief ' 2. vertoon van voorwerpe 3. gee van voorwerpe 4. uitreik: (a) nie-uitgebreid (b) uitgebreid 5. weiering: (a) nie-uitgebreid (b) uitgebreid 6. afwys: (a) nie-uitgebreid (b) uitgebreid bj tw tw tw tw md % voorkoms van kategoriee per proefpersoon 40 tw tw tw tw tw tw tw tw jf tw tw tw tw tw tw 80 jl tw tw tw 60 tw tw cw tw tw tw tw rb tw tw tw tw tw 50 40 50 vo voorkoms van kategoriee by proefpersone 83 83 50 67 50 33 50 50 50 17 tw = teenwoordig en toepaslike gebruik . die blanko spasie dui op die afwesigheid van 'n spesifieke kategorie van kommunikatiewe gebare by elke proefpersoon. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34,1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 die resultate blyk die kategoriee wat die meeste voorgekom het die kategoriee van kommunikatiewe "wys na voorwerpe" (83%) en nie-kommunikatiewe "wys na voorwerpe" (83%) te wees. volgens bates (1979) is dit een van die eerste kategoriee wat in die -vroee ontwikkelingstadia van normaal ontwikkelende kinders voorkom. die kategorie wat die minste voorgekom het is "uitgebreide afwys". slegs 17% van die proefpersone, het hierdie kategorie gebruik. hierdie kategorie vertoon ook die laagste frekwensie van voorkoms in studies wat op normaal ontwikkelende kinders deur bates (1979) uitgevoer is. die kombinasie van verbalisasies en nie-verbale kommunikatiewe gebare deur die proefpersone het minder voorgekom as wat die toepaslike voorkomsfrekwensies van die spesifieke chronologiese ouderdomsvlakke is (bates et al. 1979). hierdie vertraagde ontwikkeling van die kombinasie van nie-verbale kommunikatiewe gebare en spraakuitings mag ook die proefpersone se latere kommunikasieontwikkeling be'invloed (bates et al. 1979; long en dalston, 1982). geen ontoepaslike gebruik van enige van die kategoriee het voorgekom nie, alhoewel die gebruik van die gebare op 'n laer vlak as hulle chronologiese ouderdomme was. uit bogenoemde bespreking blyk dit dat die proefpersone vertraagde ontwikkeling van die koppeling van nie-verbale kommunikatiewe gebare met spraakuitings vertoon het. volgens long en dalston (1982) en bates (1979) kan 'n agterstand in die koppeling van nie-verbale kommunikatiewe gebare met spesifieke spraakuitings moontlik die voorloper van 'n latere ekspressiewe taalagterstand wees. hierdie konsep is deur bates (1979) en long en dalston (1982) as 'n belangrike stadium in normale taalontwikkeling gehipotetiseer maar is tot dusver beperk by ontwikkelende kinders nagevors. verkree resultate dui daarop dat dit 'n aspek van taalverwerwing is wat indringend by hoerisikokinders bestudeer behoort te word. gevolgtrekkings uit die bespreking van die resultate van die taalvorm, taalinhoud en taalgebruik van die proefpersone blyk hulle dus as 'n groep 'n ontwikkelingsagterstand te vertoon wat hulle 'ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens aanbetref, aangesien hulle ekspressiewe kommunikasievaardighede ooreenstem met die van jonger kinders (bloom en lahey, 1978; crystal et al. 1976). die grootste agterstand blyk in hierdie kinders se taalvorm voor te kom, en wel wat hulle morfologiese en sintaktiese vermoens aanbetref, aangesien hulle 'n vertraging van ongeveer ses maande vertoon. die kinders se taalinhoud vertoon die geringste ontwikkelingsagterstand deurdat slegs die gebruik van die kategorie van performatiewe 'n agterstand vertoon. die agterstand in die ontwikkeling van die proefpersone se taalgebruik blyk 'n negatiewe invloed te he op die taalvorm wat die agterstand in die ontwikkeling van die kombinasie van nie-verbale kommunikatiewe gebare en spraak van al die proefpersone aanbetref, en ook wat die teenwoordigheid van 'n negatiewe element in proefpersone cw en jl se moeder-kind-kommunikasie-interaksie betref. hierdie verskynsel kan toegeskryf word aan die wedersydse interaksie wat daar tussen taalinhoud, taalvorm en taalgebruik bestaan, soos wat deur bloom en lahey (1978) beskryf word. as gevolg van die beperkte aantal proefpersone wat vir hierdie studie gebruik is, is dit moeilik om die invloed van die h e l e n e j . . smit, brenda louw en isabel c. uys sosio-ekonomiese faktore op elke proefpersoon se taalontwikkelingsvlak te bepaal, alhoewel daar tog probeer is om verskille tussen proefpersone op te klaar. dit blyk dus dan dat die premature hoerisikokinders wat as proefpersone gedien het, se ekspressiewe kommunikasieontwikkeling van die normale afwyk deurdat dit 'n ontwikkelingsagterstand vertoon. 'n ontwikkelingsagterstand van die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens word deur verskeie outeurs in die literatuur erken. as gevolg van onvoldoende navorsing hieroor, kan daar nog nie tot 'n gevolgtrekking gekom word wat 'n voldoende verklaring vir die agterstand bied nie. 'n moontlike tentatiewe rede is dat die ontwikkelingsagterstand moontlik toegeskryf kan word aan die feit dat premature kinders probleme met integrasie ondervind as gevolg van '"n vertraging van neurologiese rypheid" wat deur die prematuriteit veroorsaak word (douglas, 1960; pasamanick en knobloch, 1960; de hirsch, jansky en langford, 1964). die beperkte aantal proefpersone van hierdie studie dien as voorloper vir verdere navorsing. aangesien slegs die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van premature hoerisikokinders in hierdie studie ondersoek is, word aanbeveel dat die reseptiewe kommunikasievermoens van premature hoerisikokinders wat afrikaanssprekend is, ook ondersoek word, ten einde 'n volledige beeld van die kinders se kommunikasievermoens te verkry. verdere studies wat die verband en onderskeid tussen die linguistiese, kognitiewe en affektiewe vermoens van hoerisikokinders ondersoek, word benodig (liebergott, et al. 1984), asook opvolgstudies om vas te stel of die agterstand in die taalontwikkeling van jong hoerisikokinders aanhou presenteer met toenemende ouderdom, asook die verhouding daarvan tot latere opvoedkundige en sosiale ontwikkeling (wright et al. 1983). die taalontwikkeling van hoerisikokinders wat jonger is as die proefpersone van hierdie studie behoort ook ondersoek te word, vir vroee identifikasie (de hirsch et al. 1964). 'n groter aantal deeglike evaluasieprosedures vir baie jong hoerisikokinders word ook benodig (liebergott et al. 1984), en ander hoerisikokinders wat kommunikasieprobleme mag ontwikkel, soos byvoorbeeld die met anoksie by geboorte, behoort ook ondersoek te word (liebergott et al. 1984; hubatch et al. 1984). i die resultate van die huidige studie beklemtoon die belangrike rol van die spraakterapeut in die vroee identifiseririg, evaluasie, behandeling en opvolg van die kommunikasievermoens van jong premature hoerisikokinders, om sodoende probleme wat mag ontstaan te minimaliseer of selfs te voorkom. aangesien voorkoming 'n belangrike rol van die spraakterapeut is, kan hoerisikokinders in hierdie opsig dus as ideale gevalle beskou word. verwysings ainsfield, m., 1984. language development from birth to three. new jersey: lawrence erlbaum associates; inc. / aram, d. & nation, j. 1980. preschool language disorders and subsequent language and academic difficulties. journal of communication disorders, 13, pp. 159—170. bates, e., 1977. language and context; the acquisition of pragmatics. new york: academic press, inc. / ' bates, e., 1979. the emergence of symbols: cognition and communication in infancy. new york: academic press, inc. benton, a.l., 1962. behavioural indices of brain injury in school children. child development, 33, pp. 199—208. the south african journal fcmmunitin disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) η ondersoek na die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van premature hoerisikokinders 31 bloom, l. & lahey, m. 1979. language development and language disorders. new york: john wiley & sons, inc. bloom, l., rocissano, l. & hood, l. 1976. adult-child discourse: developmental interaction between information processing and linguistic knowledge. congenitive psychology, 8, pp. 521-552. clezy, g., 1979. modification of the mother-child interchange in language, speech and hearing. baltimore: university park press. cole, p.r., 1982. language disorders in preschool children. new jersey: prentice-hall inc. crystal, d., fletcher, p. & garman, m. 1976. the grammatical analysis of language disability: a procedure of assessment and remediation. londen: edward arnold ltd. de hirsch, k., jansky j.j. & langford w.s., 1964. the oral language performance of premature children and controls journal of speech and hearing disorders, 29 (1, pp. 60—69). dore, j., 1978. conditions for the acquisition of speech acts, in markova, i. (ed.): the social context of language. new york: john wiley. douglas, j.w., 1960. "premature" children at primary schools. brit, med. j. 5178, pp. 1008-1013. ehlrich, c.h., shapiro, e., kimball b.d. & huttner m., 1973. communication skills in five-year-old children with high-risk neonatal histories. journal of speech and hearing research, 16, pp. 522—529. ferguson, g.a., 1981. statistical analysis in psychology and education. montreal: mograq-hill, inc. field, t.m., dempsey j.r. &schuman, h.h., 1979. developmental assessment of infants surviving the respiratory distress syndrome in field, t.m. (ed.): infants born at risk. new york: s.b. medical & scientific books. fitzhardinge, p., 1980. current outcome: i c u populations in brann, a.w. (ed.): neonatal neurological assessment and outcome. columbus, o.h.: ross, laboratories. hubatch, l.m., johnson, c.j. kirstler d.j., burns w.j. & moneka, w. 1985. early language abilities of high-risk infants. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 50, pp. 195—207. kastein, s. & fowler, e.p., 1959. language development among survivors of premature birth. archives of otolaryngology, 69, pp. 131-135. keenan, e., 1974. conversational competence in children. journal of child language, 1, pp. 163—183. liebergott, j.w., bashir, a.s. & schultz, m.c., 1984. dancing around and making strange noises: children at risk in holland, a.l. (ed.): language disorders in children. san diego california: college hill press, inc. long, n.v. & dalston, r.m.,' 1982. paired gestural and vocal behaviour in one-year-old cleft lip and palate children. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 47, pp. 403—406. long, n.v. & dalston r.m.,| 1982. gestural communication in twelve-month-old cleft lip and palate children. cleft palate journal, 19 (1), pp. 57—61. lotter, e.c., 1974. 'n ondersoek na die ontwikkeling van artikulasie by die afrikaanssprekende kind met die oog op die opstel van 'n geskikte artikulasie toets. ongepubliseerde m. logverhandeling, universiteit van pretoria. lotter, e.c., 1979. afrikaanse artikulasie-ondersoek. universiteit van pretoria. louw, b., 1986. kommunikasie-evaluasie van jong kinders 0-3 jaar. communiphon, nr. 270, in druk. louw, b., & van der merwe, α., 1981. eerste ondersoek van die orofasiale meganisme. universiteit van pretoria. lund, n.j. & duchan, j.f., 1983. assessing children's language in naturalistic contexts. n.j.: prentice-hall. marge, m. 1984. the prevention of communication disorders. american speech-language-hearing association, 26 (8), pp. 29-33. miller, j.f., 1981. assessing language production in children: experimental procedures. baltimore: university park press. northern, j.l. & downs, m.p., 1984. hearing in children. baltimore, williams & williams. pasamanick, b. & knobloch h., 1960. brain and behaviour. symposium 2. brain damage and reproductive casualty. amer. j. orthopsychiat, 30, pp. 298—305. rodgon, m., 1979. knowing what to say and wanting to say it: some communicative and structural aspects of single-word responses to questions. journal of child language, 6, pp. 81-90. rosetti, l. 1986. high-risk infants; identification, assessment and intervention. san diego: little, brown & co. smit, h.j.e., 1986. 'n ondersoek na die ontwikkeling van die ekspressiewe kommunikasievermoens van premature hoerisikokinders. ongepubliseerde b.log-verhandeling, universiteit van pretoria. strominger, a. & bashir α., 1977. a nine-year follow-up of language disordered children. paper presented at the annual convention of the american speech-language-and-hearing association. chicago, november. tyack, d. & ingram, d., 1977. children's production and comprehension of questions. journal of child language, 4, pp. 211-224. van riper, c., 1978. speech correction. principles and methods. prentice-hall, inc. weiss, c.e., lillywhite h.s. & gordon, m.e., 1980. clinical management of articulation disorders. st. louis: the c.g. mosby company. wood, b.s., 1981. children and communication. new york: prentice hall, inc. wright, n.e., thistlewaite, d., elton, r.a., wilkinson, e.m. &forfor, j.o., 1983. the speech and language development of low birth-weight infants. british journal of disorders in communication, 18 (3), pp. 187—196. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34,1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) n e w from amtronix — computerized impedance system features analog meters for compliance and pressure permit easy visualising of test. digital displays for ear canal volume and static compliance. tympanometric pressure may be read digitally. tympanometric gradient is calculated and digitally displayed. digital displays for frequency and intensity for stimuli. expanded reflex mode reveals latency, amplitude, growth and morphology. evoked potential testing with cadwell instrumentation ea sa ca e » e s \ ea ο ω s9 «a « (u μ s3 frl objective detection of a u d i o l o g i c and vestibular dysfunction the simple solution for complex audiologic cases auditory evoked response to evaluate middleand late-latency responses from the higher centers of the cortex eng to evaluate central and peripheral vestibular dysfunction bsep to objectively localize deficits to the cochlea, the auditory nerve, or the brainstem auditory pathways 40-hertz evoked response audiometry to objectively measure hearing thresholds electroneurography to obtain objective measurements of facial nerve function ί audiometry rooms i ; • \ i i ί . l μ amtronix (pty) ltd., p.o. box 630, bedfordview 2008 phone (011) 6221743 teletex 4-50033 amtron fax 6221306 i i n r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 29 the acoustic reflex at a 1000 hz probe frequency: phasor and vector analysis judy ferguson and louise reynolds department of logopaedics, university of cape town. abstract phasor plots of reflex growth functions have been inconclusive concerning the effect of the reflex for mass dominated ears. the present study aimed to establish whether vector plots clarified the effects of the reflex for phasors not showing a clear circular shape. measured admittance data (ipsilateral reflexes across a wide intensity range) was represented as both phasor diagrams and converted to impedance quantities, represented as vectors. the results analysed for 34 ears showed few unclassifiable phasor diagrams. in addition, all growth functions showed increased stiffness on vector analysis. resistance changes appeared to be variable. the results suggest that vector diagrams may be a useful way of representing data that is not clearly represented via phasor diagrams. the current study, however, does not clarify the pattern for mass dominated ears. opsomming fasor-uitstippeling van refleksiewe groeifunksies was tot nog toe onafdoende ten opsigte van die refleks se uitwerking by massa-gedomineerde ore. die onderhawige studie het as mikpuntgehad om vas te stel of vektor-uitstippeling die uitwerking van die refleks opgeklaar het ten aansien van fasore wat nie 'n duidelik sirkulere vorm vertoon het nie. gemete toegangsdata (ipsi-laterale reflekse oor 'n wye intensiteits-spektrum) is weergegee sowel as fasor-diagramme as omgesit in skynweerstandsyfers, aangebied in vektore-vorm. die uitslae ten aansien van 34 ore wat ontleed is, het weining onklassifiseerbare fasordiagramme opgelewer. bowendien, alle groeifunksies het verhoogde styfheid vertoon by vektor-ontleding. weerstandsveranderinge was skynbaar wisselend. die resultate wys in die rigtingdat vektor-diagramme moontlik 'n nuttige manier mag wees om data weer te gee wat nie duidelik deur fasor-diagramme weergegee word nie. die huidige studie klaar egter nie die patroon vir massa-gedomineerde ore op nie. key words: immittance,! reflex measures, high probe frequency, phasors, vectors introduction the middle ear acts as a transducer of sound from the external environment to the cochlea (berlin & cullen, 1980). the effects of the acoustic reflex on the transmission of sound through the middle ear can be observed indirectly through immittance measures (wiley & block, 1985). this technique has been used extensively in the literature to describe the effects of the reflex on transmission properties of the middle ear at low probe frequencies (226 and 678 hz). however, the effects of the reflex recorded at higher probe frequencies using immittance measures, have not been fully investigated in the literature. this is of particular interest given that baseline transmission properties differ across the frequency range in human ears (berlin & cullen, 1980). there are several ways of representing the quantities of the relationship between admittance and impedance components (van camp & creten, 1976). rectangular notation represents the magnitudes of both components of admittance (susceptance (b) and conductance (g)) or impedance (reactance (x) and resistance (r)) as co-ordinates (b;g) or (x;r). polar notation represents the components of admittance or impedance as having both mag/ nitude and phase angle (degree) (magnitude ; <0). dynamic aspects of middle ear function, such as reflex growth functions, may be represented as phasor diagrams, using rectangular notation (joining up the coordinates). this method has been used by lutman (1984) and reynolds and morton (1995) to investigate the effect of the acoustic reflex. vector diagrams, as used by bennett and weatherby (1979) show the dynamic aspects of the reflex by selecting points along the dynamic process and representing aspects of the reflex in both polar and rectangular notation. immittance recordings of the acoustic reflex are usually made in admittance rather than impedance components, because there is a linear relationship between the admittance components at the probe tip and the plane of the tympanic membrane (margolis, 1981). however, impedance quantities are usually used to explain immittance patterns (van camp & creten, 1976). it is possible to correct measurements made at the probe tip to the level of the tympanic membrane, and to convert these components from admittance to impedance, using the formulae shown in table 1. researchers agree that the major effect of the reflex at low probe frequencies (220 and 660 hz) is an increase in stiffness or negative reactance (feldman & williams, 1976; die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 judy ferguson and louise reynolds bennett & weatherby, 1979, lutman, mckenzie & swan, 1984; reynolds and morton, 1994). this is made clear in phasor diagrams, which show circular, anticlockwise movement. this same effect is shown as an increase in negative phase angle on vector diagrams. table 1: formulae to convert admittance (y) components to components to impedance (z) components (margolis, 1981). jx = -jb/(b2 + g2) r = g/(b2 + g2) x = reactance β = susceptance r = resistance g = conductance reynolds and morton (1995) examined whether phasor plots at 1000 hz, where normal ears may not be stiffness dominated, followed the circular, anti-clockwise phasor diagram, indicating the constant resistance, stiffness change model proposed by lutman (1984). while the majority of their phasor diagrams matched lutman's (1984) model, some of their plots were difficult to interpret, particularly those derived from ears that were mass dominated at the probe frequency used (1000 hz). the reasons for some phasor plots deviating from the standard model proposed by lutman (1984) could have been due to procedural variables, such as the nonsimultaneous recording of susceptance and conductance, or could be due to the effect of the reflex on systems where transmission properties are at or above resonance. previous researchers have found variable effects of the reflex on resistance. this variability influences the overall effect at higher probe frequencies due to smaller reactance effects which cannot so easily mask the resistance changes that are occurring, as happens for low probe frequencies (sprague, wiley & block, 1981; bennett & weatherby, 1979; and feldman & williams, 1976). it is possible that phasor diagrams are unclassifiable when significant resistance changes occur, and as they interact with reactance changes, cause irregularities in the shape of the phasor. if this were the case, then phasor diagrams may not be the clearest means to show the effect of the reflex, for high probe frequencies or mass dominated ears, but the true effect of the reflex may be clarified through representing the effect of the reflex through plotting the resistance and reactance changes as vectors. this study aimed to explore whether unclassifiable phasor representations of admittance measures could be explained by means of vector representations of impedance values derived from the same reflex measures. of further interest was whether mass dominated systems were always responsible for unclassifiable phasor plots as suggested by reynolds and morton (1995) and the present study therefore also investigated the relationship between baseline transmission and the phasor plots obtained. clarification as to whether deviations from the constant resistance, stiffness change model were related to the means of representation was therefore the focus of the study. methodology subjects thirty four normal hearing young adults served as subjects. data was collected from one ear per subject to prevent duplication of results due to the very small inter-aural differences reported in subjects by hall (1979) and creten, van der heyning and van camp (1985). subjects were required to be within 18 and 30 years of age, and to have no known history of ear pathology. normal hearing was determined as pure tone air and bone conduction thresholds within normal limits (-10 and 25 db hl), as defined by goodman (1965, cited by yantis, 1985). air and bone conduction thresholds were required to be within 10 db of each other to exclude any middle ear pathology not known to the subject. in addition, normal middle ear functioning was required, and this was established on the basis of tympanometry and acoustic reflex measurements. subjects were required to have a single peaked admittance tympanogram at 226 hz probe frequency, and ipsilateral acoustic reflex thresholds within normal limits (70 1 0 0 db hl), as defined by northern, gabbard and kinder (1985), for 500, 1000 and 2000 hz stimulus frequencies, measured at a 226 hz probe frequency. apparatus hearing thresholds for all subjects were established using either a gsi10 clinical audiometer, with tdh-50p headphones and b71 bone vibrator, or a beltone 2000 audiometer, with tdh-50p headphones and b71 bone vibrator. both audiometers are calibrated in hearing level and meet the ansi s.26-1981 standard for clinical audiometers. immittance measurements were carried out using a gsi-33 (version 2) middle ear analyser, which meets the ansi s.3.39-1987 standard for acoustic-immittance instruments. the instrument was calibrated for the specific altitude of the test environment (98 m above sea level). calibration checks, following the manufacturer's instructions, were carried out on each day of the data collection. all hearing threshold measurements were carried out in acoustically treated audiometric suites (lac 109), meeting the sabs 0182 (1982) code of practice. procedure baseline information for each subject was established first in order to relate phasor and vector classifications to transmission at 1000 hz. baseline transmission at 1000 hz for both susceptance (b) and conductance (g), recorded simultaneously, was established for each ear according to the vanhuyse, creten and van camp (1975) classification. tympanometric values were recorded at pressure' values of-350 dapa, and tympanometric peak, in order to (correct the values to the plane of the tympanic membrane (shanks, wilson and cambron, 1993). a positive-to-negative (+200 dapa to -400 dapa) pressure sweep procedure was used. pressure was varied at a rate of 50 dapa per second (grason-stadler, 1987). shanks and lilly (1981) and margolis, van camp, wilson, and creten (1985) found that positive to negative pressure sweeps result in less complex tympanometric patterns than negative to positive pressure sweeps. acoustic reflexes were recorded at stimulus frequencies of 500 hz and 1000 hz at a 1000 hz probe frequency, for susceptance, followed by conductance, as these measures could not be displayed simultaneously. the intensity range used was 66-106 db hl. a 4 db increment size was used. this increment size has been used in other studies (for example, reynolds and morton, 1995). the the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the acoustic reflex at a 1000 hz probe frequency: phasor and vector analysis 31 m i l l i m h o change (magnitude of the reflex growth) at each intensity level was recorded as well as the direction of the reflex growth (i.e., whether it was positive or negative). the stimulus duration was kept constant at 1.5 seconds, as time related aspects were not being investigated in this study. susceptance recording always preceded conductance recordings, and measurements at 500 hz preceded measu r e m e n t s at 1000 hz. each subject's testing was completed within one day. data organisation and data analysis: data was corrected to the plane of the tympanic membrane by subtracting the susceptance and conductance values at -350 dapa from those values at tympanometric peak, as suggested by shanks et al. (1993). observed reflex values (x) were added to the corrected baseline values (bc;gc) for each intensity level: (be + x; gc + x). frequency counts of the number of phasor plots classified as above were recorded in table form. vector diagrams: all corrected susceptance and conductance values were converted to reactance and resistance values by means of the formulae presented in table 1 (margolis, 1981). these corrected and converted reactance and resistance values were plotted on vector diagrams, which allowed for examination of the impedance data. vectors were classified as showing an increase in stiffness (+s), a decrease in stiffness (-s), or no change in stiffness (os), and an increase in resistance (+r), a decrease in resistance (-r), no change in resistance (or) or variable resistance (vr) at threshold and suprathreshold levels. from this, the effects of the reflex on reactance (increase or decrease in stiffness) and changes in resistance could be extracted and presented in table form. phasor representation: corrected susceptance and conductance values were represented in the form of phasor diagrams for each reflex growth function recorded. in such diagrams, conductance values are represented on the x-axis and susceptance values on the y-axis. a phasor trajectory was obtained by joining up the coordinates at each point and the shape of the trajectory was then classified. the axes used for each graph were standard for all graphs, but the scaling for each graph was different. this was done in order to maximize the visual representation of each phasor plot. the resulting phasors were classified as either: 1. fitting with the lutman (1984) model of an anti-clockwise circular movement, indicating constant resistance, and an increase in stiffness. these were termed classifiable. 2. phasor plots not fitting the lutman (1984) model. these were termed unclassifiable. results and discussion the distribution of ears used in the study across types of baseline characteristics is shown in table 2. as expected, the majority of ears were at resonance at this frequency, with only a small number of ears being stiffness dominated. the ten mass dominated ears at the 1000 hz probe frequency were of particular interest, given the focus of the study. as normal ears are expected to be close to resonance at 1000 hz (colletti, 1977), it would be necessary to use a higher probe frequency to measure the reflex for many mass dominated ears, and the ten were considered to be a sufficient number to clarify the research question. table 3 shows, that similarly to reynolds and morton (1995), the majority of phasor plots obtained were matched to the constant resistance, stiffness change model explained by lutman (1984). an example of this is provided in figure 1. table 2: summary of | baseline transmission characteristics of subjects in this study. (n=34). ! number of subjects van huyse classification stiffness dominated eairs ! 4 1b1g ears at resonance \© 3b1g mass dominated ears 10 3b3g table 3: results of the phasor analysis of growth functions obtained for 500 and 1000 hz stimuli (n = 68). classified phasors unclassified phasors stifness dominated ears 6 2 ears at resonance 34 6 mass dominated ears 1 1 8 2 co ο jc ε ε ω «μ* ω ο c « α ω ο <λ co 1.62 1.53 1.44 1.35 1.26 1.17. 1.08 0.99 0.90 0.81 4.99 5.04 5.09 5.14 5.19 5.24 5.29 5 3 4 5.39 conductance (g) m m h o s figure 1: example of classifiable phasors demonstrating anticlockwise, circular movement form baseline to suprathreshold levels. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 judy ferguson and louise reynolds however, an unexpected result was the distribution of unclassifiable phasors across ears, regardless of baseline transmission properties. unlike reynolds and morton (1995), who found that those phasors that deviated from the model were from mass dominated ears, the results of the present study indicated that unclassifiable phasors were obtained from stiffness dominated ears, ears at resonance, and mass dominated ears. both the present study and the reynolds and morton (1995) study did not differentiate between degrees of mass domination within subjects. it is possible that the ears in the present study behaved similarly to ears at resonance or stiffness domination with the added stiffness effect of the reflex, which, as shown below, was demonstrated for the reflex patterns obtained. given that normal ears are expected to be close to resonance at 1000 hz, it may be necessary to use a higher probe frequency to demonstrate the effect of the reflex for mass dominated ears, although problems will be encountered in the measurement of such small immittance changes in mass dominated ears (lutman, 1995). of particular interest in the results of this study, is the nature of the deviation from the model. clearly, as shown in the examples in figure 2, the deviations from the constant resistance, stiffness change model are not marked in the present study, and all showed overall patterns that could be broadly described as circular, anticlockwise movement, at least for some portion of the phasor plot. these results are shown in table 4, where a brief description of the phasors obtained is provided. it is evident from this table that the patterns which were not strictly showing anticlockwise movement with the activation of the reflex typically showed clockwise movement close to threshold, and then at suprathreshold levels the typical pattern of anticlockwise movement was seen. examples of this are provided in figure 2a. one contributing factor to this pattern may relate to the determination of reflex threshold, as it is possible that the threshold values obtained were not the actual thresholds of the subjects, and that true threshold was reached at higher intensity levels, where the pattern was consistent with.the model. the criteria used to determine threshold on the gsi 33, derived from bennett and weatherby (1979), may not be absolute threshold values. lutman (1984) argues that there is some question regarding the accurateness of the criterion values given by grason-stadler (1987). it is therefore possible that absolute thresholds were not obtained at the level at which they were recorded. this would mean that only two phasors did not actually match the model (see table 4). these two demonstrated deviations from the model at suprathreshold intensity levels. an example is shown in figure 2b. it is well documented that the reflex reaches saturation (wilson and mcbride, 1978), and it may be that the complex interaction of immittance components, coupled with saturation of the reflex contributed to the patterns obtained. in analyzing the results of the study using vector analysis, an attempt was made to clarify the effect of the reflex, particularly for phasors which were not classifiable. in spite of the few unclassifiable phasors obtained, the vector analysis did provide some useful information, shown in tables 5,6 and 7, particularly as regards the unresolved table 4: description and frequency count of unclassifiable phasors (n=10) classifiable at threshold, & unclassifiable at suprathreshold levels 2 unclassifiable at threshold, & classifiable at suprathreshold levels 8 conductance (g) mmhos 4.85 5.02 520 5.38 5 56 5.74 5.92 6.10 6.28 6.46 0.55ο 0 β5 jc. i 0.75 m 0 . 8 5 w φ ο 0.95 ια <0 q. 105 φ § 1 1 5 " u5 h 1.351.62 co 1.59 ο -c ε ε m a> ο c ra a. a> υ co 3 co 1.56 1.53 1.50 1.47 1.44 1.41 1.38 1.35 a 244 2 5 0 2 5 6 2 6 2 2 6 8 274 2 8 0 2 8 6 2.92 conductance (g) mmhos figure 2: examples of unclassifiable phasors. a) represents clockwise movement at threshold, followed by anticlockwise circular movement. b) represents anticlockwise movement at threshold, followed by deviations from the model at suprathreshold levels. / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the acoustic reflex at a 1000 hz probe frequency: phasor and vector analysis 33 question of the effect of the reflex on resistance. c o n s i s t e n t with all previous studies , for example l u t m a n et al. (1984) and reynolds and morton (1995), is that the effect of the reflex on reactance is an increase in stiffness. during the course of some of the reflex growth f u n c t i o n s obtained in the present study , there was some d e v i a t i o n from this pattern at threshold, but as mentioned above, this may have related to the definition of threshold i n t e r e s t i n g l y , two growth functions, both obtained from mass dominated ears showed decreased stiffness at s u p r a t h r e s h o l d levels, as shown in table 5. however, these growth functions were classifiable within the model, as this pattern did not emerge for the unclassifiable phasors, as shown in the detailed analysis in table 7. thus, there did not appear to be any relationship between the reactance change and the classifiability of phasors. a variety of effects on resistance were observed, consistent with previous findings (for example sprague et al., 1981). as shown in table 6, the most common effects were decreased resistance at threshold, with most demonstrating increased resistance at higher intensity levels. several of these typical patterns were also obtained from u n c l a s s i f i a b l e phasors (see table 7), thus indicating that there was no clear relationship between the resistance pattern and the classifiability of phasors. conclusions while the reactance and resistance changes from classifiable and unclassifiable phasors across ears with differing baseline properties were not differentiated in the results of this study, some interesting observations relating to phasor and vector analysis of reflex growth functions were nonetheless evident. as the complex interaction of reactance and resistance change was demonstrated in unclassifiable phasors, isolating the effects of the reflex on impedance quantities does allow for the identification of increased stiffness, in spite of fairly obscure phasor patterns, possibly making this a useful tool for clinical interpretation of obscure reflex patterns. however, identifying expected patterns for mass dominated ears appears to require either a more sophisticated form of recording reflex phenomena, or more sophisticated form of analysis. factors contributing to the lack of clarity of the effect may i table 5: vector analysis: ̂ effect of the reflex on stiffness reactance. j +s = increase in stiffness -s = decrease in stiffness ^ os = no change in stiffness ; = shows the difference between baseline, threshold and suprathreshold levels. stiffness dominated ears ears at resonance mass dominated ears +s 8 24 7 -s;+s 0 10 11 os;+s 0 6 0 +s;-s 0 1 0 2 relate to the nonsimultaneous recording of the reflex, but probably the most overriding factor is the use of a probe frequency where normal ears are typically at resonance. thus in order to clarify the effect of the reflex for mass dominated ears, a higher probe frequency should be incorporated for studies of normal ears, but this is not currently possible using commercially available equipment, due to the increasing complexity of immittance recordings as ears deviate from simple stiff systems. one further table 6: vector analysis: effect of the reflex on resistance. +r = increase in resistance -r = decrease in resistance or = no change in resistance vr = variable resistance changes ; = shows the difference between baseline, threshold and suprathreshold levels. stiffness dominated ears ears at resonance mass dominated ears or;+r 3 2 0 -r;+r 2 21 11 +r 0 0 4 or;-r 0 2 0 -r 2 7 2 vr 0 4 1 or 1 0 0 -r;or 0 4 2 table 7: detailed vectorial description of the ten unclassifiable phasors. +s = increase in stiffness -s = decrease in stiffness os = no change in stiffness +r = increase in resistance -r = decrease in resistance or = no change in resistance vr = variable resistance changes ; = shows the difference between baseline, threshold and suprathreshold levels. stiffness dominated ears ears at resonance mass dominated ears +s, or;+r os;+s, -r;+r -s;+s -r;+r +s, -r;+r +s, -r -s;+s -r;+r os;+s, vr +s, -r;or -s;+s, -r +s, -r;+r die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 judy ferguson and louise reynolds possibility may be to investigate the pattern of reflex growth functions in pathological ears, or in ears selected because of their marked mass domination at the probe frequency, rather than in terms of simply a normal phenomenon, as was the case in the present investigation. references american national standards institute. (1987). specifications for instruments to measure aural acoustic impedance and admittance. ansi s 3. 39. new york, ansi. american national standards institute. (1981). specifications for audiometers. ansi s 26. new york, ansi. bennett, m.j, & weatherby, l.a. (1979). multiple probe frequency acoustic reflex measurements. scandinavian audiology 8 233239. berlin, c.j, & cullen, j.k. (1980). the physical basis of impedance measurement. in jerger, j & northern, j, (eds.) clinical impedance audiometry. (2nd ed.) american electromedics corp. colletti, v. (1977). multifrequency tympanometry. audiology. 16 278-287. creten, w.l, van der heyning, p.h, & van camp, k.j. (1985). immittance audiometry: normative data at 220 and 660 hz. scandinavian audiology. 14 115-121. feldman, a.s, & williams, p.s. (1976). tympanometric measurement of the transmission characteristics of the ear with and without the acoustic reflex. scandinavian audiology. 5 43-47. grason-stadler. (1987). gsi33 middle ear analyser: preliminary instruction manual. grason-stadler international, inc. hall, j.w. (1979). effects of age and sex on static compliance. archives of otolaryngology. 105 153-156. lutman, m.e. (1984). phasor admittance measurements of the middle ear 1: theoretical approach. scandinavian. audiology. 13 253-264. lutman, m.e, mckenzie, η & swan, i.r.c. (1984). phasor admittance measurements of the middle ear 2: normal phasor tympanograms and acoustic reflexes. scandinavian audiology. 13 265-274. lutman, m.e. (1995). personal communication. margolis, r.h. (1981). fundamentals of acoustic immittance. in popelka, g.r. (ed). hearing assessment with the acoustic reflex. grune and stratton. margolis, r.h, van camp, k.j, wilson, r.h, & creten, w.l. (1985). multifrequency tympanometry in normal ears. audiology. 24 43-54. northern, j.l, gabbard, s.a, & kinder, d.l. (1985). the acoustic reflex. in katz, j (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology. (3rd ed.) williams & wilkins. reynolds, l. & morton, l.p. (1994). direction of susceptance and conductance reflexes at high probefrequencies. the south african journal of communication disorders. 41 85 92. reynolds, l. & morton, l.p. (1995). acoustic reflex measures at a high probe frequency: a phasor diagram approach. british journal of audiology 29 144 152: shanks, j.e, & lilly, d.j. (1981). an evaluation of tympanometric estimates of ear canal volume. journal of speech and hearing research 24 557-566. shanks, j.e, wilson, r.h, & cambron, n.k. (1993). multiple frequency tympanometry: effects of ear canal volume on static acoustic admittance and estimates of middle ear resonance. journal of speech and hearing research 36 178-185. south african bureau of standards. (1982). code of practice for obtaining an acoustic environment suitable for audiometric testing. sabs 0182. sprague, b.h, wiley, t.l, & block, m.g. (1981). dynamics of acoustic reflex growth. audiology. 20 15-40. van camp, k.j, & creten, w.l. (1976). principles of acoustic impedance and admittance. in feldman, a.s, & wilbur, l.a. (eds.) acoustic impedance and admittance the measurement of middle ear function. williams & wilkins. vanhuyse, v.j., creten, w.l. & van camp, k.j. (1975). on the w-notching of tympanograms. scandinavian audiology. 4 45 5 0 . wiley, t.l., & block, m.g. (1985). overview and basic principles of acoustic immittance measurements. in katz, j. (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology. (3rd ed.) williams & wilkins. wilson, r.h. & mcbride, l.m. (1978). threshold and growth of the acoustic reflex. journal of the acoustical society of america. 63 (1) 147 164. yantis, p.a. (1985). pure tone air conduction testing. in katz, j (ed.) handbook of clinical audiology. (3rd ed.) williams & wilkins. ι the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 42, 1995 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 the problem of stuttering: where are we in 1986?* hugo h. gregory, ph.d (northwestern university, usa) department of communication disorders, northwestern university, illinois, usa ttaler regies the author's experience as a person with a stuttering problem, as a student and as a professional clinician, teacher and researcher. an overview of the state of the art in 1986 is presented z7rl7v7skaf-noorsigvandieouteurseondervindingas-nhakkela^ van hakkel. die stand van die teorie en die kliniese praktyk in 1986 word beskryf my review and analysis, leading to statements about the current status of our knowledge of stuttering, begins over 40 years ago when a blond, fourteen year old boy from the state of arkansas in the usa journeyed 1500 miles to the state of rhode island to get help with his stuttering problem. that summer at an institution known as martin hall, i learned that stuttering was due to a faulty reaudiorization and revisualization of words and developed fears of speaking. therapy consisted of being on silence (no conversation) for periods of time in which we practised syllables, words and sentences from a drill manual. we learned a rule for the production of each consonant, and as we said a syllable or word, we thought of the rule for each consonant and called up strong auditory and visual images. this first step was known as word analysis. i analyzed words all over that beautiful landscape on mount hope bay in rhode island. in word analysis, transitions between sounds were very smooth, but words were spoken one at a time. at the end of two weeks, we came off a silence for the weekend and were allowed to speak, using careful word analysis. to a girl, to whom i had been writing notes while on silence, i was now able to say, "p-a-t, w-o-u-l-d y-o-u l-i-k-e t-o g-o t 0 t-h-e m-o-v-i-e s-a-t-tj-r-d-a-y n-i-g-h-t?" after that weekend we went back on silence to practise phrasing in which we analyzed only the first word of the phrase. at the end of two more weeks, i was able to say, "pat / would you like to go / to the biltmore hotel / in providence / for dinner / saturday night?". in addition to the improvement in speech, the jump up in two weeks from a movie in bristol to dinner at the biltmore in providence was pretty exciting. not bad for a 14 year old! this was my first introduction to what we now designate as the speak-more-fluently approach to therapy. i viewed word analysis and phrasing as the way to break the habit of stuttering and to learn to speak fluently. i hoped to substitute word analysis and phrasing for stuttering. although i was conscientious in practising words and sentences every day, utilizing my rules, a few months after i returned home i began to slip, to have increasingly more trouble. however, i never had as much difficulty again as before that first summer of therapy. today, we would say that i did not have an adequate transfer and maintenance experience. during this period, i gained "this paper is a written version of the ρ dev pienaar memorial lecture die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 a greater appreciation of the fear component of stuttering, as i perceived the way in which my apprehension about speaking reappeared. when i returned to the program the followingsummer, i began to realize that i had concentrated on the speech aspect of therapy and was missing a great deal of the part having to do with attitude. i recalled that the clinicians had talked about the way in which stutterers, in fact all people, tend to overemphasize what they perceive as a problem. for example, stutterers tend to become very sensitive about the fluency of their speech. in examining my attitude, i began to see that if i stuttered in a situation, i was very hard on myself. i considered myself pretty much of a failure. later on, after i was in college, wendell johnson's ideal helped me to understand that i should not attempt to evaluate myself as "either or", i.e. either i am a stutterer or i am not a stutterer. therefore i began to view myself more and more as a person who stuttered sometimes as he talked, and i realized that i was going through a process of changing. in 1949, i went to northwestern to study "speech correction". i learned about many theories of stuttering with the most focus on the ideas of charles van riper and wendell johnson. van riper spoke of predisposing factors, precipitating factors, and maintaining factors. predisposing factors could be physiological. johnson emphasized that the misevaluation of children's disfluency by parents and others led to an apprehensive, anticipatory, hypertensive, avoidance reaction. this led to the learning of stuttering. i had some great experiences. i met and talked with wendell johnson and lee edward travis at the 1950 convention of the american speech and hearing association attended by only 400 people. i had some not-so-great experiences too. i met a gentleman at that convention who asked me what i had learned about stuttering at northwestern, and so i told about bryngelson, johnson, sheehan, travis, van riper, etc. he asked if i had heard of isaac karlin's delayed mylinization theory; and after i said i had not, he told me, "i am isaac karlin." of course, i reviewed van riper's text and found out about dr. karlin and wrote to tell him i now knew about his theory. well, you live and learn! delivered on 16 july 1986 at the university of the witwatersrand. © sasha 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) at that 1950 convention of asha, john black from ohio state and grant fairbanks of the university of illinois demonstrated delayed auditory feedback (daf) equipment and the effect of daf on speech. one of my professors introduced me to them and i spoke under daf. apparently i did better than most people, i.e. daf did not distrub my speech flow as much as black and fairbanks expected. they concluded that this was due to my concentrating on tactile-kinesthetic monitoring as i spoke, one thing i had learned to do in therapy. as the 1950s moved along, several studies including my own doctoral dissertation, looked at auditory processes in stutterers, research that has continued until the present time. findings of a problem in the auditory system have not been conclusive. in the decade and one-half from 1960 to 1975, there was a great emphasis on studying stuttering as learned behaviour. actually, the foundation for this work was laid by wischner's reasoning, relating learning theory concepts to johnson's diagnosogenic explanation of the development of stuttering, and by sheehan's writing about stuttering as an approach avoidance conflict. from sheehan and wischner, i learned that the momentary reduction of anxiety and tension that follows the occurrence of stuttering probably reinforces unadaptive stuttering behaviour (sheehan, 1953; wischner, 1950). since we were so focused upon the punishing nature of stuttering, it was difficult to comprehend how the occurrence of stuttering could be rewarding or reinforcing in this way. in my first university teaching position at southern illinois university, from 1958-1962,1 had two colleagues, israel goldiamond and eugene brutten, both of whom were to become well known for their research relating learning concepts to stuttering. flanagan, goldiamond, and azrin (1958) investigated the possibility that stuttering was an operant behaviour by presenting bursts of a loud noise contingent upon moments of stuttering. this contingency resulted in decreases of stuttering in 3 subjects. later they presented five seconds of daf contingent upon stuttering with the same results. they began to conclude that stuttering responded like an operant behaviour as defined by skinner (1953). flanagan related to me that one day while he went to get coffee, he left the daf on continuously, and on his return, he found a stutterer speaking without stuttering in a prolonged speech pattern. thus, the use of daf in therapy was born. after many more studies showing that the effects of assumed positive, negative and neutral verbal response contingencies ("right", "wrong", "tree") and time-out were all equally as effective in reducing stuttering, it was assumed that the best explanation was that all of these contingencies highlighted stuttering. highlighting, it was said (siegel, 1970) may result in an increase of the aversive properties of cognitive and response produced stimuli; thus, these responses could serve to punish the behaviour. maybe in this way, stuttering did follow the rules of operant behaviour. meanwhile, returning to my sequential story, my other colleague at southern illinois university, gene brutten, had teamed up with don shoemaker to offer us a two-factor theory of stuttering (brutten and shoemaker, 1967). their concept was that fluency, the predominant characteristic of normal speech, is disrupted by learned classically conditioned negative emotionality that increases fluency failures (repetitions and prolongations). unadaptive instrumental behaviours or operant responses, known commonly as secondary reactions, that reduce emotional' responses are reinforced, adding to the complexity of fluency failure. a very important observation to be made of conditioning studies hugo h. gregory was that not all subjects responded in the same way. again saw that stuttering could not be simplified. in a d d i t i o n ^ emotional variables and overt behavioural variables, c o m p i ° cognitive variables probably also influence stuttering behaviou * designing research to deal with all of these influences is conmr cated and requires much more work. l~ as for the application of learning concepts, hobart mowrer the great american psychologist, assured me that we speech-languat^ pathologists had always been the quintessence of behaviourists'1 during the period i am now discussing, we clinicians, influenced by the behaviourists of our times, gave more specific attention to target responses, instructions, modelling, reinforcement, and the programming of change. we became more definitive in measurino behaviour and recording quantity of change in our reports i never thought we could measure objectively all of the dimensions of change with which we deal, but we measured what we could and our goal has been to strive for better approaches to assessment. meanwhile, kidd and his associates (kidd, 1980), and even sheehan (sheehan and costley, 1977), were becoming more convinced that there is a genetic factor in stuttering, a postulation that has been around for a long time. at the same time, all of us were recognizing the higher incidence of other speech and language problems in children who stutter (an observation first made by mildred berry [1938]). perhaps, it is the motolinguistic developmental factors that are inherited. finally, a book clarifying the present status of our knowledge about the genetic aspects of speech and language disorders has been written (ludiow and cooper, 1983). more recently, the findings of brain hemispheric functioning differences with reference to alpha suppression when anticipating expressive speech has brought forth considerable discussion. stutterers show more alpha suppression (representing greater activity) in the right hemisphere on verbal tasks; whereas nonstutterers show more in the left hemisphere (moore, 1984). this difference reminds us of the differences in auditory dichotic listening (using meaningful words) in which stutterers show more reversals and smaller between ear difference scores (gregory and mangan, 1982). remember, these are group results and do not mean that all stutterers respond in one way or another. finally, the recent findings that a group of adult stutterers who showed more right hemisphere alpha suppression before therapy, shifted to more left hemisphere suppression following therapy is intriguing. the therapy program emphasized the temporalsegmental aspects of speech, and possibly this treatment is mirrored by brain functioning (boberg, yendall, schopflocher and bolassen, 1983). that is the way it should be. although findings are somewhat mixed, the motor speech reaction time differences in which stutterers show slower voice initiation times implies a slower reacting motor system in stutterers. one study of this in children (cullinan and springer, 1980) showed that stuttering children with other language and learning problems showed the motor reaction time differences, others did not. but, there is still debate about the influence of emotional conditioning on the small differences found. / here we are in 1986. the blond boy of earlier years turned grey early as he grappled with the problem of stuttering. up to this point in this paper i have traced my experience as a person with a stuttering problem, as a student, and as a professional clinician, teacher, and researcher. at this point in my development, where do i think we are? the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) problem of stuttering: where are we in about the disfluency of children's speech and , we know mor , q u t a c h i l d s disfluency with reference to when to be c o n c e r n _ ^ d i s f l u e n c y _ including such charact h e q u a n t i t y and qua > fons p e r i n s t a n c e g f part-word, or teristics as the nurnoe ^ n £ w i n f o r m a t i o n from 0 n e s y ' l a b l % u c h a f electro-g.ottography (conture, .984) will p r o c e d u r e s sucn ^ t h e d e c i s i o n making process. if provide a d j l t l ° " ^ e r e i i v i n g today, we would see that we have s ^ s ' g n s t ' y to our understanding of the complexities of children's disfluencies. , w s in the future we may be precise in stating the cause 2 . sometimes in the ^ ^ ^ ^ , e a m i n g m o r e e a c h y e a r o r causes of s utter, g a i ^ ^ ^ a p p e a r t q b e a b 0 u c i a t ^ with smttering and about how the manipulation of a s s o c i a t e d witn t t e r i n g . w e must constantly question zszzjx** ~ w e h a v e a s f e c t i v e l y a s w l mv colleagues and i are continuing to refine our pro^ fotd.ff ent.al evaluation differential theory. we are focustng our attention on adults as we.1 as children. at present, here is more specificity in this regard about children, who are nearer to the age of onset, than there is for adults. 3 we have gained a better understanding of motor and linguistic factors associated with stuttering and about how to manage those factors as an integral part of therapy. many stuttering children appear to have marginal capacities for speech production. the demand for communication has to be controlled carefully. with references to research findings that there are minimal motoric differences of the speech mechanism (slowness of vocal reaction time and longer voice onset times [vot] or measurably longer glottal adduction per glottal cycle in some children who stutter) it would seem appropriate to utilize procedures that provide' a vivid model for the child and that usually involve the slowing of speech production and a smoother blending of speech sound transitions. on the other hand, if we are only dealing with maladaptive learning, these modifications of speech production are still appropriate. possibly, we should consider giving stutterers practice in the rapid initiation and termination of vowels and consonant-vowel combinations. assuming that the fluency of speech is related to these basic skills, particularly when children are experiencing linguistic or environmental stress, it could be important to improve these skills, j 4. progress has been made in assessing parent-child interaction factors more objectively using standardized observation procedures. recent work by meyers and freeman (1985a,b) on parental interruptions of disfluent children is important in this regard. parallel to this we are counselling parents more effectively by employing not only verbal counselling, but by modelling changes for them. i first wrote about this in 1973, and we have continued to refine procedures over the years for changing parental behaviour, as well as the child's behaviour. 5. in the past 15 years, research and clinical experience in behaviour-analysis behaviour-modification has enabled us to be more effective and efficient in changing the behaviour of children who are beginning to stutter, their parents, and more confirmed stutterers. we are more systematic in the manipulation of stimulus variables including consequences for unadaptive and adaptive behaviour. but the planned use of modelling, clinicians seeing the importance of their role as a model, has been one of the most significant developments in behaviour change procedures. behaviour modification is viewed to include making up for deficit behaviours such as sound sequencing and word finding, assertive behaviour, and social skills. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 6 the controversy between the advocates of stutter-more-fluently and speak-more-fluently models has been clarified and many clinicians are now integrating the two. for example, we begin with adult stutterers by monitoring unadaptive stuttering behaviour, stuttering with less tension; and then, through relaxed speech onsets, phrasing, etc. stutterers learn behaviour that is counter to stuttering. a major advantage of combining the two models is that stutterers learn to cope with moments of stuttering, resulting in reduced sensitivity about stuttering and diminished fear of regression or relapse, but they also learn improved speech skills and flexibility in speaking. 7. during the last ten years we have faced up to the definite need for planned transfer and maintenance activities that include making speech production spontaneous and natural. in this connection, in building fluency i always attempt to distort natural prosody as little as possible. i now say that the person, child or adult has to continue therapy on a less intensive basis, with less and less self-monitoring, for 12-18 months following the core period of therapy (whether it is one month, six months, or whatever), until speech is more spontaneous and normally fluent, and self-confidence to handle variations in fluency and disfluency is sufficient. 8. attitude change remains a controversial area. valid measurement is a paramount issue. possibly a new scale being standardized by jenifer barber watson that taps behavioural, cognitive, and affective factors and that stresses a comparison of these components will be useful. all clinicians influence attitudes. some see direct work on cognitive and affective components as important others see behaviour change as the most effective approach to attitude change. although subjective, i believe we need to explore beliefs and feelings as verbalized, or demonstrated in behaviour, to help clients clarify their attitudes and see the relation between their attitude and their behaviour. 9 increased measurement of stuttering behaviour, and here we still have a problem of definition, has resulted in more efficient and effective therapy. criteria for stuttering differ somewhat, but one syllable word repetition, part-word sound and syllable repetition, prolongations, combinations of these behaviours, and unique vocal tract disruptions characteristic of individual stutterers are counted as stuttering. extratherapy measurements involve problems. overt and covert measures, according to howie woods and andrews (1982), appear to differ the most immediately following treatment. ingham (1984) has provided a model for a time-series of assessments before, during, and following therapy. 10 clinicians are becoming more able to adapt procedures, generate their own approaches, and treat stutterers as the individuals they are. 11 finally, we must improve the training of clinicians. more clinicians are specializing in stuttering therapy. in the summer of 1985 we had a two-week workshop for specialists who already had from one to twenty years of experience. i would be pleased to see the certification of specialists so that the public could identify those who are more competent in the evaluation and treatment of stutterers. (continued on page 7) r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aids for • the development of perception • the acquisition of speech and language skills • the improvement of motor co-ordination plus • helpful texts for therapists • educational toys, books and equipment • records for auditory training • catalogues on request • large variety of tests available stockists of • learning to listen • two sound lottos • full lda range play and schoolroom 44 president place, 148 jan smuts ave., rosebank. telephone 788-1304 p.o. box 47288, parklands 2121. ο the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 7 the problem of stuttering (continued from page 5) references berry, m. developmental history of stuttering children. j. ped., 12: 209-217 1938. brutten, e. and shoemaker, d. the modification of stuttering. new jersey, prentice-hall, 1967. boberg, e., yendall, l., schopflocher, d., bo-lassen, p. the effects of an intensive behavioural program on the distribution of eeg alpha power in stutterers during the processing of verbal and visuospatial information. j. fluency dis., 8, 245263, 1983. conture, e. observing laryngeal movements of stutterers. in curlee, w. and perkins, w.h. (eds). nature and treatment of stuttering. san diego: college-hill, 1984. cullinan, w. and springer, m. voice initiation and termination times in stuttering and nonstuttering children. / speech hear. res., 23, 344-361, 1980. flannagan, b., goldiamond, i. and azrin, n. operant stutterin the control of stuttering behaviour through response contingent consequences. j. experimental analysis ofbehav., 1, 173-177, 1958. gregory, h. and mangan, j. auditory processes in stutterers. in lass, n. (ed) speech and language advances in basic research and practice, volume 7. new york: academic press, 1982. howie, p., woods, c. and andrews, j. relationship between covert and overt speech measures immediately before and immediately after stuttering treatment. j. speech hear. disorders, 47, 419-422, 1982. ingham, r. toward a therapy assessment procedure for treating stuttering in children. in gregory, h.h. (ed). stuttering therapy: prevention and intervention with children. memphis: speech foundation of america, 1984. kidd, k. genetic models of stuttering. j. fluency dis., 5,187-201, 1980. ludlow, c. and cooper, j. genetic aspects of speech and language. new york: academic press, 1983. meyer, s. and freeman, f. interruptions as a variable in stuttering and disfluency. j. speech hear. res., 28, 428-435, 1985a. meyers, s. and freeman, f. mother and child speech rates as a variable in stuttering and disfluency. j. speech hear. res., 2.8, 436-444, 1985b. moore, w.h. central nervous system characteristics of stutterers. in curlee, w. and perkins, w.h. (eds). the nature and treatment of stuttering. san diego: college-hill press. sheehan, j.g. theory and treatment of stuttering as an approachavoidance conflict. journal of psychology, 36, 27-49, 1953. sheehan, j.g. and costle, m. a reexamination of the role of heredity in stuttering. j. speech hear. dis., 42, 47-59, 1977. siegel, g.m. punishment, stuttering and disfluency. j. speech hear. res., 13, 677-714, 1970. skinner, b. the sciences of human behaviour. new york: appleton-century-crofts, 1953. wischner, g.j. stuttering behaviour and learning: a preliminary theoretical formulation. j. speech hear. dis., 15, 324-335, 1950. the application of the peabody picture vocabulary test — revised (ppvt-r) to non-mainstream children erna alant, d.phil (pretoria) s.m. beukes, ma (pretoria) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of pretoria. abstract this article focuses on the difficulties involved in diagnosing the communication of non-mainstream speakers within a south african context. various alternatives are discussed whereby tests can be made more relevant to this population. the revised version of the peabody picture vocabulary test is applied to a group of afrikaans-dialect speaking coloured children in order to determine the merits of standardizing the jest for this population. the test results are correlated with the accuracy scores on a story. the findings indicate the feasibility of the standardization of the ppvt-r for this population. opsomming die problematiek verbonde aan die diagnosering van nie-standaarsprekers se kommunikasie in 'n suider-afrikaanse konteks word onder die loep geneem verskeie alternatiewe benaderings waardeur die relevansie van toetse verhoog kan word, word bespreek. die hersiene weergawe van die peabody picture vocabulary test is op 'n groep nie-standaard afrikaanssprekende kleurling kinders toegepas ten eindediemerietevan toetsstandaardisasie vir hierdie teikengroep te bepaal. die toetsresultate word met die akkuraatheidstellings van 'n storie gekorreleer. die bevindings dui daarop dat standaardisasie van die ppvt-r vir hierdie populasie uitvoerbaar is. © sasha 1986 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykmgs, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sajcd 256 are we there yet? on a journey towards more contextually relevant resources in speech-language therapy and audiology audiologists and speech-language therapists working in developing contexts like south africa have the opportunity to address a range of needs through their research. one of these needs is the development of assessments and therapy materials that are appropriate for their clients’ particular language needs and cultural background. this editorial paper aims to introduce original research in speech-language therapy and audiology, which has been carried out in south africa and other developing contexts and is presented in this volume of the journal. in addition we suggest that while the focus of much research is on the destination or end product that is developed, there is a need to share the methodologies that are used to reach that goal so that more research can be carried out by a wider pool of students, researchers and clinicians. we describe some of the methods that we have used in our research – often in small scale projects with budgetary constraints, which would be feasible for clinicians in their routine clinical contexts. our hope is that others can build on these approaches, critique and share their own strategies for the ultimate advancement of the professions in southern africa. keywords: cross-cultural adaptation, methodology, resources, delphi, isixhosa, afrikaans s afr j cd 2013;60:2-9. doi:10.7196/sajcd.256 appendices a c are available in the pdf version of this article online at http://dx.doi.org/10.7196/sajcd.256 speech-language therapy and audiology are relatively new and relatively small professions in south africa and are beset with a host of challenges. some of these challenges include perceived low priority and low relevance against the backdrop of a quadruple burden of disease, lack of awareness about the profession and its scope, and the fact that language and communication lie at the heart of our work; yet in many cases the mismatch between therapists and audiologists and their clients in terms of language creates almost insurmountable barriers in providing equitable service delivery. many papers that have been published in this journal (barrat, khoza-shangase & msimang, 2012; kathard, naude, pillay & ross, 2007; pascoe & norman, 2011; penn, 2007) and in others (penn, 1998; southwood & van dulm, 2013; swanepoel, 2006; maphalala, pascoe & smouse, 2013) detail this context and the challenges of providing a population-based service delivery. authors such as kathard et al. (2007) and penn (2007) focused on the need for more research, as well as research that has greater relevance in our context. barrat et al. (2012) looked primarily at clinical encounters and the use of untrained personnel as translators and interpreters. southwood and van dulm (2013) focused on development of multilingual therapy materials, and maphalala et al. (2013) on isixhosa speech assessment. one of the more tangible aspects of the challenges faced is that of a lack of contextually relevant assessment and therapy materials. in 2011 our editorial introduction (pascoe & norman, 2011) explored this in detail, while that entire volume (sajcd, volume 58) showcased original south african research that was carried out with a view to furthering the development agenda of speech-language therapy and audiology. in this volume of sajcd, gonasillan, bornman and harty (2013) describe their work with toddlers from a range of different language and cultural backgrounds in south africa and the way in which the language development survey (lds) (rescorla, 1989), a widely used checklist with demonstrated validity and reliability, was adapted and found suitable for use within the south african context. detailed and systematic studies of established assessments, such as this one, provide much-needed support for clinicians who may be unsure about which assessments are appropriate for adaptation, and where it may be preferable to devise an assessment ‘from scratch’. another excellent example of contextually relevant intervention material comes from southwood and van dulm and (2012) who have devised a language therapy tool kit specifically for use with older children in the south african context who speak english and/or afrikaans. they presented this at the recent saslha conference held in bloemfontein in september 2013. of course, materials development or adaptation is not the only priority for the professions in south africa. however, the focus on materials development is appealing because it provides a very tangible and practical focus for speech-language therapists (slts) and audiologists (as) who want to make a change to their practice with clients and who may wish to become involved in research that will have a direct bearing on their own clinical practice. in this volume, the papers by wium and louw (2013) and erasmus, schutte, van der merwe and geertsema (2013), speak to the need for the professions to respond to the changing contexts in which we work. other priorities for the professions include finding and documenting the research designs and methodologies employed. ‘pragmatic approaches’ have been described in the literature as research which has relevance to stakeholders and is feasible to be conducted in and used in real-world settings (glasgow & riley, 2013). there is a growing literature base describing pragmatic study designs and the development of pragmatic measures. pragmatic measures are clearly much needed in our fields. single case studies are another potentially useful research design relevant to developing contexts. in the editorial introduction to child language teaching and therapy, vance and clegg (2012) suggested that the single case study can make an important contribution to the evidence base of the profession of speech-language therapy. one of the main messages here was that case studies are achievable for practitioners in their everyday working contexts ‘of nurseries, schools and other health and educational services’ (p. 257). vance and clegg’s editorial inspired our current editorial paper; we believe that practitioners have an important role to play in the adaptation and development of materials for use with all our clients in south africa. the pragmatic approaches and methodologies such as single case studies are achievable for practitioners in their everyday work settings. the challenges that we face in south africa are not unique to our context. mcleod, verdon and bowen (2013) recently published a paper detailing the development of a position paper to guide slts in managing children’s speech in multilingual contexts. in this paper, the five phases that were followed in order to develop the position paper are detailed: a face-to-face workshop, creation of the initial draft, online panel discussion, thematic analysis, moderation and finalisation. the paper was developed by 57 members of the international expert panel on multilingual children’s speech who represented 33 different countries. of course, the final product will be of great interest to slts and as working in southern africa (http://www.csu.edu.au/research/multilingual-speech/position-paper) and has close relevance to the topic under discussion here. what is also interesting and inspiring about mcleod et al.’s (2013) paper is the careful detailing and description of the way in which the expert panel was formed and the journey that was undertaken to reach the ultimate destination of the position paper. it is a journey that could be undertaken by different groupings in different contexts to address different issues. we aim to describe some of the research methods that can be used by clinicians to add to the body of contextual knowledge. the work we describe is not necessarily an example of best practice, but rather modest examples aimed to inspire and empower practitioners. replications of small-scale studies across different settings and different individuals will increase the external validity of these projects. ultimately publication should occur to ensure that the work is shared and read by practitioners. all too often development projects have fallen by the wayside because the ultimate destination was not reached, and valuable methodological lessons are lost. the methodologies described here could all be used in a variety of different projects and adapted for the particular aim of a study. they all require few resources and are simple to execute. they are ‘pragmatic’ in the sense of being important and relevant for stakeholders as well as being timeand cost-effective. the delphi technique is a research approach that has been widely used in social sciences, and increasingly in health sciences (keeney, hasson & mckenna, 2011). it involves the formation of an expert panel or panels whose members have specific knowledge of a particular topic. the panel is tasked with reaching consensus on a particular topic or set of questions. multiple rounds are often needed in order for the panel to carry out a problem-solving or decision-making remit. originally developed for military settings in which the demands of a task exceeded a single person, the delphi technique has been widely adapted for a range of very different purposes. in our research we have used this approach in two different ways: (i) to reach consensus regarding the selection of stimuli (words and pictures) which were needed for use in a pilot version of an isixhosa speech assessment for children, and (ii) to reach agreement about the translation of items for an audiology assessment. maphalala (2012) convened a delphi panel as part of a larger study which aimed to develop a single-word-picture naming assessment in order to collect preliminary data about the typical development of isixhosa speech (see maphalala et al., 2013). the objectives of the panel were to arrive at consensus regarding the stimuli (single words and pictures) chosen for the assessment. the panel was presented with a draft list of words. the process was undertaken to ensure that all words included were (i) culturally appropriate, (ii) age-appropriate for the children in question, and (iii) that the assumed pronunciation and meaning were also correct. there were five participants on the panel chaired by the researcher. two participants (females) were first-language isixhosa speakers. they were preschool educators who are familiar with young children. two participants (one male, one female) were academics at the university working in african languages. these participants were not first-language isixhosa speakers but were fluent in that language and had studied it. both of them are parents of young children. finally, the fifth participant was an undergraduate student of speech therapy who had isixhosa as her first language. the panel was chosen because of their innate and learnt knowledge of isixhosa and because of their interest and knowledge of young children’s speech and language development. a checklist was devised for the purpose of the panel (table 1). it contained words that had been selected by the researcher and were thought to include all the speech sounds (consonants and vowels) and word shapes that appear in the isixhosa phonetic inventory. the words were judged to be age-appropriate for preschool children as well as able to be represented by a picture. however, these judgments of the researcher required further validation from the panel. the purpose of the study was explained and checklists were distributed. an explanation of the word-checking process was given. terms were defined (e.g. what is meant by culturally appropriate) and participants were given the opportunity to ask questions about the checklist. participants were given time to fill out the checklist, a process that took approximately 20 minutes. they were urged to refrain from any discussions until the checklists were completed. during this time, no audio recordings were taken. recording began when the discussion was started. in order to analyse the wordlist, each word was read out aloud by one of the first-language speakers. participants were given the opportunity to comment on words they found inappropriate, i.e. if a word received any ‘no’s’, it was considered problematic and was discussed. participants who raised issues with words were asked to comment on what the exact problems were. this was discussed among the group and if there was agreement, a new word was suggested by the panel. this was checked in terms of the three criteria and if it passed, it was then used as part of the wordlist. of the 65 words initially in the list, 54 were found appropriate in all three categories. the main reason for inappropriate words was that they were not age-appropriate vocabulary. table 1 provides examples of some of the problematic items. the development of a preliminary isixhosa speech assessment was a challenging process that required ongoing validation and development. using the expert panel at this early stage of the research was helpful in validating some of the decisions that had been made, and also in flagging potentially problematic items. another example of the use of a delphi panel comes from audiological literature and illustrates how this type of approach can be used in translation work in our field. rogers et al. (2011) focused on dizziness, a common occurrence in the general and medical population. frequently patients and clinicians are at odds regarding symptom definition, e.g. there are 26 different english language words for ‘dizziness’, not all of which suggest the quality of the complaint and especially not a possible vestibular origin. lack of understanding becomes compounded when managing symptom description across a range of languages. the original english language vertigo symptom scale ((vss), yardley, masson, verschuur, haacke & luxon, 1992) is a validated self-assessment scale, which explores aspects of vertigo and associated symptoms of anxiety, and has been widely used in research and clinical settings. translated measures should be validated; however, there is evidence in the literature that both the quality and validation of translated instruments are variable (sousa & rojjanasrirat, 2011). delphi panels offer a time-efficient, low-cost and effective way to reach consensus and refine translations of instruments. panels may be facilitated electronically which may offer an additional advantage in that members do not influence one another in the way that may occur in focus groups – as in maphalala’s (2012) project, for example; but instead can be used to build on the work of all members and reach consensus (mokkink et al., 2010). rogers et al. (2011) used a modified (two-round) delphi consensus procedure to refine translation of the vss, as well as to seek opinion regarding afrikaans words which would capture the essence of the word ‘vertigo’. two first-language afrikaans-speaking audiology students independently translated the vss into the target language. thereafter two panels were selected in preparation for the delphi consensus procedure. panel 1 comprised five lay individuals who were first-language afrikaans speakers. levels of education ranged from having completed high school (grade 12) to university graduates. five bilingual healthcare professionals who regularly treated patients with vertigo contributed to panel 2. disciplines included audiology, otolaryngology, aviation medicine and psychology. with the exception of the psychologist, all practitioners had received specialised post-graduate training in vestibular disorders and were familiar with the vss. the initial two translations into afrikaans were merged into one as there were no differences between the versions. translation back into english helped highlight areas of ambivalence, e.g. items in which a choice of words could be used, which in turn formed the basis for discussion with both expert panels. questionnaires with five-point likert-scale responses and space for qualitative comments regarding the translation of the vss were prepared for each panel. participants in panel 1 responded to questions regarding the language, grammar and vocabulary of translated items. examples of questions are shown in table 2 and specifically probe if the meaning of translated items is clear. questionnaires were constructed with both positively and negatively phrased constructs in order to cross-check consistency of opinion. the purpose of round one of the delphi was to: • identify items on which there were high levels of agreement regarding the translation. these could be adopted immediately without need for further exploration. • give and elicit examples of different words and terminology for the translation from english to seek vocabulary equivalence in afrikaans. vocabulary equivalence (suleiman & yates, 2011) involves the selection of words, which capture the essence, and the nuances of the word used in the original. • identify which items were problematic. these items and the suggestions that emanated from them were fed back to both panels in round two. panel 2’s focus was on the applicability of the translated items to the afrikaans-speaking patient population. in addition, panel 2 was invited to contribute words commonly used by their patients to describe their symptoms of dizziness. the questionnaire used for panel 2 is shown in appendix a. the forward-backward method of translation proved effective with the proposed afrikaans version submitted for round one of the delphi, reaching high levels of consensus with both panels. items which required further refinement were identified by both panels and the combined input from lay and professional groups fed back in round two allowed a tight focus and an abbreviated version of the delphi to achieve reliable results when the afrikaans vss was presented to a patient population (rogers et al., 2011). input from a variety of disciplines allowed development of a vocabulary which would be recognised by first-language afrikaans speakers, patient populations and clinicians. the panels described by maphalala (2012) and rogers et al. (2011) were relatively simple to organise and manage; participants brought a variety of different types of expertise to the groups (different professional backgrounds, parental, tertiary education/academic and preschool education) so that a wealth of information was obtained, and in the case of maphalala’s group (2012) lively debate occurred. the checklists and questionnaires were helpful in structuring and focusing the experts. weaknesses include the relatively small number of participants in the groups, the fact that not all participants were first-language speakers of the language in question and that the dialects and origins of speakers were not explicitly considered. authors such as keeney et al. (2011) and cialkowska, adamowski, piotrowski and kiejna (2008) caution that delphi approaches have been revised and adapted so many times that they can end up being very different watered-down forms of the rigorous approach originally intended. for example, cialkowska et al. (2008) note that the original delphi was characterised by features which include anonymity of responses from panel experts, controlled feedback and a statistical description of responses. the size of the panel used in the research by maphalala (2012) and rogers et al. (2011) is also small, compared to the recommended number which literature suggests may range from 10 to over 1 000 participants. however, it should be noted that the literature is clear that increased panel size does not necessarily improve the decision-making process or guarantee a more valid outcome (powell, 2003). the size of the panel may well be dependent on the expertise that is available on a given topic at a given time. clearly maphalala’s (2012) panel may not meet all the characteristics of a true delphi – she referred to it as an ‘expert panel’. the boundaries between a true delphi panel and an expert panel convened for pragmatic purposes may be blurred in this type of research. this should not matter if the panel serves its purpose. in contrast to the delphi panels described, the process of protocol development involves documenting the methods used in research as the primary aim in order to enable replication in further projects or clinical settings. fish et al. (2012) devised a protocol for the development of speech processing and production tasks with young children. working with isixhosa-speaking children, they found that there were few assessment resources available in this language, and in order to answer their research questions, they needed to develop some isixhosa assessment tools suitable for their participants. since the process of devising these assessment materials proved complex, they documented the steps taken and the practical requirements so that others in a similar situation looking to develop stimuli for a specific language, would be able to build on the steps they had taken. the protocol follows the principles of the stackhouse and wells (1997, 2001) psycholinguistic framework, and the stimuli selection and design principles detailed as part of this framework. in glasgow and riley’s (2013) paper describing pragmatic measures, they suggest that it is desirable for pragmatic approaches and measures to be related to a theoretical model, which can help in understanding and interpreting findings and ultimately advance scientific understanding. the protocol details three tasks: naming, repetition and auditory discrimination. the children who participated in this study were three 2-year-old children who were acquiring isixhosa as their first language and attended a crèche in nyanga, cape town. the results of the tasks carried out are described by fish et al. (2012). the protocol is presented in figure 1. fig. 1. a protocol for assessing speech processing and production in young children (from fish et al., 2012). the single-word naming list (from maphalala, 2012) was used as the starting point for the development of the tasks. children were asked to name simple pictures of everyday items such as ball (ibhola), head (intloko), cat (ikati) and money (imali). where children could not name words because of vocabulary limitations, they automatically defaulted on the naming task and responses were elicited using a repetition format. items that were spontaneously named were analysed in terms of speech accuracy and how closely they resembled adult target productions. additional words were then selected for the repetition task, and these included a mix of words that were accurately and inaccurately produced. children may struggle with repetition tasks for a variety of reasons and having different stimuli to repeat (unknown words which are effectively non-words, words which a child knows but cannot yet produce, and words which a child knows and can produce in an adult-like way) may help a clinician to determine the nature of the underlying difficulties. the auditory discrimination task used an abx protocol, a simplified auditory discrimination task appropriate for 2-yearolds. this type of task does not require the participants to understand the concepts of ‘same’ and ‘different’ as many tests of auditory discrimination require. in the abx task, children were confronted with their own error and the correct target production, and asked to discriminate between the two using a visual format. for example, if the child produced ‘imati’ for ‘ikati’, the abx task may have involved these two words. one toy would ‘say’ the word ‘imati’ (stimulus a) and another toy the word ‘ikati’ (stimulus b). the child would then be asked to show which toy had said one of the words, e.g. ‘imati’ (stimulus x). appendix b is a form that could be used to administer the assessment tasks; it provides a framework to record the data from the three tasks in a manner that allows for ready administration and analysis. items that are not named immediately default to the repetition task, and auditory discrimination items are only based on errors in the naming and repetition tasks. data can then be transferred to appendix c, which provides a structure for analysis based on what was done in this study. the protocol presented here could be used to assist with clinical assessment of children with speech difficulties – when working in any language. additional resources may be needed to complete the protocol, e.g. a phonetic inventory of the language in question. using three tasks allows for more in-depth investigation of children’s speech processing, stored representations and speech production skills than typical ‘output only’ tasks allow. children may present with similar speech sound difficulties that reflect different underlying causes, e.g. one child may have articulation or motor programming difficulties, and another child whose speech errors appear similar may have difficulties with auditory discrimination which means that words are inappropriately stored and produced as a result of the input problems. having detailed information about a child’s speech processing, production and phonological representations may mean that if intervention is needed the slt can make sure it is specific to the child’s needs. some children will require therapy focusing on input skills and others may need help in updating old/inaccurately stored motor programmes. it is not suggested that this is the only or even best way to undertake this work, but fish et al. (2012) aimed to share what was done as a documented starting point. authors such as these contribute to the development agenda of slts in south africa by documenting their journey undertaken and making it available to others who may find it useful for their own research or clinical purposes. conclusions in this editorial we aimed to share some of the methods that we used in research projects that have aimed to adapt or develop contextually relevant resources for the use of slts and as in the western cape region of south africa. delphi panels can be used in a variety of ways to meet a variety of goals. here we gave two examples of how they could be used to assist with stimuli selection when compiling assessment or therapy tasks, and the process of translation. these panels can enhance the validity of the work undertaken in studies of cross-cultural adaptation and are well documented in the literature (beaton, bombardier, guillemin & ferraz, 2000; du plessis & human, 2007; keeney et al., 2011; vernon, 2009; world health organization, 2012). we aimed to describe them specifically as they were used in studies focusing on slt/a materials development in south africa. the results of some small-scale or single-subject research may be limited in generalisability to the wider population, but we have argued that there may be much to be learned about the journey taken – even if the ultimate destination is not yet reached. the answer to the question posed in our title is probably ‘no’ – we have some way to go in our development of culturally and linguistically appropriate materials for use with all of the people with communication difficulties in our country. however, the journal shows that we are moving forward towards this goal. small projects, undertaken by clinicians in response to their day-to-day needs, and the work of undergraduate students in their clinics, assignments and research projects, need to be ‘out there’ for all to see, share and learn from. documenting the steps taken, such as the protocol of fish et al. (2012), is one way in which a small-scale study can make a potentially bigger impact. acknowledgements. thanks to anita edwards for managing the peer review of this paper, and to the peer reviewers for their helpful comments. thanks to our undergraduate student groups who contributed to some of the work we describe; zinhle maphalala and mantoa smouse for their key roles in much of the isixhosa work described here, and shajila singh who was involved in discussions which led to this paper. m pascoe, phd; c rogers, msc audiology; v norman, m communication pathology department of health and rehabilitation sciences, university of cape town barrat, j., khoza-shangase, k. & msimang, k. (2012). speech-language assessment in a linguistically diverse setting: preliminary exploration of the possible impact of informal ‘solutions’ within the south african context. south african journal of communication disorders, 59, 34-44. doi:10.7196/sajcd.57 beaton, d. e., bombardier, c., guillemin, f., & ferraz, m. b. (2000). guidelines for the process of cross-cultural adaptation of self-report measures. spine, 25 (24), 3186–3191. cialkowska, m., adamowski, t., piotrowski, p. & kiejna, a. (2008). what is the delphi method? strengths and shortcomings. psychiatr pol, 42 (1), 5-15. du plessis, e. & human, s. p. (2007). the art of the delphi technique: highlighting its scientific merit. health gesondheid, 12, 13-24. fish, l., jansen, c., manley, n., powell, m., pratt, k. & rosen, l. (2012). speech processing and production in two-year old children acquiring isixhosa: a description of three cases. unpublished undergraduate thesis. university of cape town. glasgow, r. & riley, w. (2013). pragmatic measures: what they are and why we need them? am j prev med, 45(2), 237-243. kathard, h., naude, e., pillay, m. & ross, e. (2007). improving the relevance of speech-language pathology and audiology research and practice. south african journal of communication disorders, 54, 5-7. keeney, s., hasson, f. & mckenna, h. (2011). the delphi technique in nursing and health research. chichester: wiley-blackwell. doi:10.1002/978144439 maphalala, z. (2012). phonological development of first language xhosa-speaking children aged 3;0 5;11 years: a descriptive cross-sectional study. unpublished undergraduate thesis. university of cape town. maphalala, z., pascoe, m. & smouse, m. (2013). phonological development of first language isixhosa-speaking children aged 3;0 6;0 years: a descriptive cross-sectional study. clinical linguistics and phonetics, early online: 1-19. doi: 10.3109/02699206.2013.840860. mcleod, s., verdon, s., bowen, c. & the international expert panel on multilingual children’s speech. (2013). international aspirations for speech-language pathologists’ practice with multilingual children with speech sound disorders: development of a position paper. journal of communication disorders, 46: 375-387. mokkink, l. b., terwee, c. b., patrick, d. l., alonso, j., stratford, p. n., et al. (2010). the cosmin study reached international consensus on taxonomy, terminology and definition of measurement properties for health-related patient reported outcomes. j clin epidemiol, 63, 737-745. pascoe, m. & norman, v. (2011). contextually relevant resources in speech-language therapy and audiology in south africa – are there any? south african journal of communication disorders, 58, 2-5. penn, c. (1998). the study of child language in south africa. folia phoniatrilogop, 50, 256-270. penn, c. (2007). ‘don’t give me the theory, just tell me what to do in therapy!’: the slippery slope challenge for the south african professions of speech-language pathology and audiology. south african journal of communication disorders, 54, 13-17. powell, c. (2003). the delphi technique: myths and realities. j adv nurs, 41(4), 376-382. rogers, c., de wet, j., gina, a., louw, l., makhoba, m., & tacon, l. (2011). the translation of the vertigo symptom scale into afrikaans: a pilot study. south african journal of communication disorders, 58, 6 – 12. rescorla, l. (1989). the language development survey: a screening tool for delayed language in toddlers. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 54, 587-599. sousa, v. & rojjanasrirat, w. (2011). translation, adaption and validation of instruments or scales for use in cross-cultural health care research: a clear and user-friendly guideline. journal of evaluation in clinical practice, 17, 268 – 274. southwood, f. & van dulm, o. (2012). receptive and expressive activities in language therapy (realt). johannesburg: jvr psychometrics. southwood, f. & van dulm, o. (2013). child language assessment and intervention in a multilingual context: the state of the art in the rainbow nation. paper presented at saslha conference, bloemfontein, september 2013. http://www.entcongress.co.za/images/2013presentations/venue3/30th/14h30southwoodvandulmvenue3monday.pdf stackhouse, j. & wells, b. (1997). children’s speech and literacy difficulties i: a psycholinguistic framework. london: whurr publishers. stackhouse, j. & wells, b. (2001). children’s speech and literacy difficulties ii: identification and intervention. london: whurr publishers. suleiman, k. h. & yates, b. c. (2011). translating the insomnia severity index into arabic. j nurs scholarsh, 43, 49-53. swanepoel, d. (2006). audiology in south africa. international journal of audiology, 45, 262-266. vance, m. & clegg, j. (2012). use of single case study research in child speech, language and communication interventions. editorial introduction. child language teaching and therapy, 28(3) 255–258. vernon, w. (2009). the delphi technique: a review. international journal of therapy and rehabilitation, 16 (2):69-76. world health organization. (2012). process of translation and adaptation of instruments. retrieved on 22 november 2012 from http://www.who.int/substance_abuse/research_tools/translation/en/ yardley, l., masson, e., verschuur, l., haacke, n. & luxon, l. (1992). symptoms, anxiety and handicap in dizzy patients: development of the vertigo symptom scale. j psychosom res, 36, 731-741. 'ν neuro-evolusionere εν neurofisiologiese benadering tot die ondersoek en behandeling van verworwe disartrie anita van der merwe m.a.log. (pretoria) departement spraakheelkunde, universiteit van pretoria opsomming die konvensionele benadering tot verworwe disartrie soos dit gevind word in die literatuur word kortliks bespreek. die noodsaaklikheid vir 'n gewysigde benadering wat praktiese leidrade aan die spraakterapeut sal verskaf, word beklemtoon. die opkoms van die neuroevolusioner-gefundeerde benadering tot serebrale verlamming, met ander woorde, tot kongenitale disartrie, word genoem. sover bekend, is daar geen georganiseerde gegewens in die literatuur wat die neuro-evolusie van spraakmotoriek blootle vanaf die mees outomatiese, mees georganiseerde en mees eenvoudige vlak tot die meer willekeurige, meer komplekse, en minder georganiseerde vlak van funksionering nie. bekende gegewens in verband met spraak is hipoteties georden en 'n hierargiese spraakskema is opgestel wat die neuro-evolusie van spraak blootle. op grond van die skema en met inagneming van neuro-fisiologiese disfunksies, is 'n ondersoek program opgestel wat die spraakprobleem van die verworwe disartriegeval in sy geheel ondersoek. behandeling geskied aan die hand van die riglyne wat die skema aandui. deur die gebruik van toepaslike spraakterapeutiese metodes word beoog om 'n herevolusie te bewerkstellig waar moontlik. summary the conventional approach to acquired dysarthria as found in the literature, is briefly discussed. the necessity for an altered approach, which will provide practical guidelines for speech therapists, is emphasized. the approach to cerebral palsy (in other words to congenital dysarthria) based on neuro-evolutional theory, is mentioned. as far as is known, there are no organised data in the literature that describe the neuro-evolution of s p e e c h motor-co-ordination from the most automatic, most organized and simplest level to the more voluntary, more complex and less organized level of functioning. known data concerning speech have been hypo the tically ordered and a hierarchical speech scheme describing the neuro-evolution of speech was constructed. based on this scheme and taking the neurophysiological dysfunction into account, an examination procedure which examines acquired dysarthria as a whole, has been compiled. therapy follows the guidelines indicated by the scheme. disartrie is 'n griekse woord wat letterlik beteken 'n versteuring van artikulasie as gevolg van 'ri letsel van die perifere of sentrale senuweestelsel.18 die term behels egter meer as dit en verwys na defektiewe motoriese beheer van die totale spraakmeganisme uitgesonderd die simboliese. dit verwys na spraakprobleme as gevolg van paralise, swakheid, of swak koordinasie van spraakspiere en dit sluit in artikulasie, fonasie, respirasie en resonansie.5'10 die term "verworwe" dui aan dat die toestand nie aangebore is nie, maar dat gestabiliseerde spraak reeds bestaan het voor die toestand ingetree het. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 anita van der merwe aanduidings van belangstelling in verworwe disartrie word in die literatuur gevind vanaf die begin van hierdie eeu. navorsing op die gebied volg veral twee benaderingswyses. enersyds word een of meer van die spraaksimptome wat kenmerkend is van verworwe disartrie, ondersoek. enkele simptome soos byvoorbeeld hipernasaliteit,19 verstaanbaarheid,15 wanartikulasie, disdiadokokinese,14 en verminderde beweeglikheid van artikulators,7 word in isolasie bestudeer. die verband met ander simptome en die onderliggende neuro-fisiologiese disfunksie word dus nie duidelik toegelig nie. andersyds is die simptome wat in 'n spesifieke toestand voorkom, nagegaan. baie aandag is geskenk aan parkinsonisme, veelvuldige sklerose en myasthenia gravis. verskeie nie-spesifieke terminologie is gebruik om die spraaksimptome te beskryf, byvoorbeeld "stadig", "geforseerd", "eksplosief" en "staccato". beskrywing van 'n sekere toestand se simptome verskil van outeur tot outeur en die gevolg is 'n algemene verwarrende beeld. dieselfde neiging om die probleem oppervlakkig te benader, word gevind met betrekking tot die behandeling van verworwe disartrie. metodes wat wissel vanaf die koumetode,17 tot die gebruik van eksterne hulpmiddels byvoorbeeld 'n prostese vir nasale spraak,16 en chirurgiese kleefband,4 word beskryf. elk van die metodes kan wel nuttig wees deurdat sekere simptome sodoende verlig word. geen algemene beginsels word egter in die literatuur verskaf op grand waarvan al die simptome van alle gevalle van verworwe disartrie ondersoek en behandel kan word nie. darley en sy medewerkers,5'6 doen in 1969 die mees omvattende studies tot op datum. spraaksimptome van al die verskillende tipes verworwe disartrie word bestudeer met as doel die samestelling van differensiaal-diagnostiese simptoomgroepe. die simptome word verklaar en in verband gebring met die onderliggende neurofisiologiese disfunksie. die praktiese implikasies van die studies is egter steeds beperk vir die spraakterapeut. gedurende die vyftigerjare is 'n groot deurbraak gemaak op die gebied van serebrale verlamming toe bobath en bobath neuro-evolusioner georienteerde terapiemetodes in fisioterapie begin toepas het. mysak11 het die waarde van die benadering besef en dit ook toegepas op die behandeling van spraakprobleme van die serebraalverlamde kind. mysak12 verklaar egter dat die enkele groot struikelblok in die weg van die praktiese toepassing van 'n neuro-evolusionere benadering in spraakterapie, die gebrek aan georganiseerde materiaal op die gebied van oro-neuro-motoriese ryping van spraak, is. kennis van die gebied is noodsaaklik aangesien die motoriese probleme van die serebraalverlamde vanuit 'n neuro-evolusionere oogpunt gesien word as 'n arrestasie of wanontwikkeling op die evolusieleer van motoriek. die simptome van verworwe disartrie kan daarenteen gesien word as 'n devolusie van motoriek. die herverskyning van infantiele orale reflekse by die volwasse brein-beseerde persoon soos beskryf deur, o.a. brain,2 en mysak,13 bevestig die aanname. ten einde die rasionaal van 'n neuro-evolusioner-gefundeerde benadering te verstaan, is dit nodig om die konsep van evolusie en devolusie van die funksionering van die sentrale senuweestelsel en die duplekse simptomatologie van neuropatologie kortliks te beskryf. die welbekende neuroloog, john hughlings jackson, het in die vorige eeu hierdie beginsels gepostuleer, en dit bied steeds journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η benadering tot verworwe disartrie 47 'n handige basis vir die bestudering van die simptome van neuropatologie. jackson9 onderskei 'n hierargie in die sentrale senuweestelselareas en ook in die funksies van die areas. onderskeid word hoofsaaklik gemaak tussen hoogste en laagste vlakke. die funksies van die laagste vlak is minder kompleks of meer eenvoudig, meer georganiseerd en dus minder veranderbaar, en ook meer outomaties. bewegings wat op die vlak beheer word, toon min kompleksiteit en min spesialisasie. dit is kenmerkend van gedrag net na geboorte. die funksies van die hoogste vlak en gedrag wat beheer word op die vlak, is minder georganiseerd, meer kompleks en meer willekeurig. mysak13 definieer die term "evolusie" as: ' . . . an ascending development in a particular order'. die genoemde onderskeid in funksies dui dus ook die orde van motoriese ontwikkeling aan. ontwikkeling vind plaas vanaf die mees outomatiese funksies na die meer willekeurige, vanaf die mees eenvoudige na die meer komplekse en vanaf die mees georganiseerde, tot die funksies wat steeds verder georganiseer word.1 3 daar is dus 'n ordelike verloop in motoriese ontwikkeling. elke vlak van ontwikkeling is die resultaat van die vorige vlak en die voorloper van die daaropvolgende vlak.3 primitiewe motoriese response word geleidelik gei'nhibeer namate hoer vlakke van ontwikkeling bereik word.1 neuropatologie veroorsaak daarenteen 'n devolusie of afbraak van funksionering en gee aanleiding tot 'n duplekse simptomatologie volgens jackson.9 negatiewe simptome is die verlies van funksies as gevolg van 'n letsel terwyl positiewe simptome die resultaat is van aktiwiteit of ooraktiwiteit van die intakte dele van die sentrale senuweestelsel wat vrygestel is van beheer deur hoer dele, 'n voorbeeld van 'n negatiewe simptoom is 'n parese of paralise wat die gevolg is van 'n verlies van willekeurige beheer en 'n voorbeeld van 'n positiewe simptoom is die verskyning van infantiele reflekse.13 in terme van die leer van devolusie is die negatiewe element dus die verlies van die minder georganiseerde, meer komplekse en meer willekeurige funksies, terwyl die positiewe element die vrylating is van meer georganiseerde, meer eenvoudige en meer outomatiese funksies. die verskynsel van duplekse simptomatologie bevestig dus die konsep van devolusie van motoriek as gevolg van neuropatologie. indien die neuro-evolusionere beginsels van sentrale senuweestelselfunksies toegepas word op spraakmotoriek, impliseer dit 'n rangorde van kompleksiteit van spraakgedrag wat geleidelik tot stand kom en ook afgebreek kan word as gevolg van breinskade van die motoriese areas. indien sodanige rangorde of evolusieleer saamgestel kan word, kan dit dus 'n raamwerk bied waarbinne verworwe disartrie ondersoek en behandel kan word. die mate van devolusie kan nagegaan word en die heropbou van spraakfunksies kan geskied aan die hand van die riglyne wat die evolusieleer, of soos dit later genoem word, die hierargiese skema, aandui. alvorens daartoe oorgegaan word om die neuro-evolusionere beginsels toe te pas op spraak, moet genoem word dat die meer neurofisiologiese aspekte van verworwe disartrie nie geignoreer mag word nie aangesien dit van groot belang is in die behandeling. motoriek en dus ook spraakmotoriek word beheer deur die gesamentlike werking van 'n aantal dele van die sentrale senuweestelsel. in tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 anita van der merwe die verskillende etiologiese groepe of ook genoem motoriese siridroomgroepe, word motoriek op 'n kenmerkende wyse aangetas. 'n letsel van enige deel van die sentrale senuweestelsel wat motoriek bei'nvloed, sal waarskynlik 'n devolusie van spraakgedrag tot gevolg he aangesien komplekse spraakfunksies afhanklik is van die gesamentlike werking van die verskillende dele van die sentrale senuweestelsel. sover moontlik word die neurofisiologiese disfunksie, byvoorbeeld 'n parese wat die uitvoer van meer of minder komplekse funksies verhoed, dus behandel ten einde 'n herevolusie te bewerkstellig. waar beide die neuro-evolusionere en neurofisiologiese komponente in ag geneem word, bied dit 'n verantwoordbare rasionaal vir die ondersoek en behandeling van verworwe disartrie. die toepassing van neuro-evolusionere beginsels op spraak sover bekend bestaan daar geen georganiseerde gegewens wat die neuro-evolusie van spraak blootle nie. in terme van die genoemde beginsels kan afgelei word dat ook spraakmotoriek vanaf'n mees outomatiese, mees georganiseerde en mees eenvoudige vlak na 'n meer willekeurige, minder georganiseerde en meer komplekse vlak van funksionering ontwikkel. die infantiele orale reflekse kan gesien word as die laagste vlak van orale funksionering en as die beginpunt van vegetatiewe gedrag en spraakontwikkeling. ingram8 se dat die reflekse plaasvind via die medulla en op latere stadiums in willekeurige motoriese aktiwiteite gei'ntegreer word. oor die verband wat daar bestaan tussen spesifieke orale reflekse en artikulasie-beweging kan slegs gespekuleer word. gestabiliseerde spraak is direk geinisieerde en willekeurige bewegings, maar die presiese funksionering is afhanklik van die onderliggende onbewuste integrasie van die aktiwiteite van 'n komplekse reeks reflekse. hierdie reflekssisteme verskaf die onbewuste substratum vir spraakproduksie.20 die reflekssisteme word gekodrdineer met die willekeurige meganismes deur die dalende projeksiebane vanaf die serebrale korteks, nl. die kortikobulbere en kortikospinale bane.21 enkele reflekse kan waarneembaar uitgelok word in die spraakorgane, nl. die faringale, palatale en mandibulere reflekse. die reflekssisteme kan beskou word as die meer outomatiese, meer georganiseerde en meer eenvoudige aspekte van orale funksies tydens spraakontwikkeling en gestabiliseerde spraak, en word dus onderskei van die infantiele reflekse wat as mees outomaties geklassifiseer is. die vegetatiewe funksies, die velofaringale sfinkter, respirasie en fonasie word op grond van hul funksionering op 'n laer bewussynsvlak deur die skryfster gegradeer as meer outomatiese, meer georganiseerde "en meer eenvoudige funksies. as 'n derde vlak, word die ontwikkeling van artikulasie en artikulasie tydens gestabiliseerde spraak, as die meer willekeurige, minder georganiseerde en meer komplekse aspek van: orale funksionering, onderskei. daar kan dus tot die slotsom gekom word dat gestabiliseerde spraak gegradeerde komponente bevat, met ander woorde, outomatiese en willekeurige aspekte. genoemde gradering dui dus nie slegs die orde van ontwikkeling aan nie, maar gee 'n aanduiding van die neuro-evolusionere organisasie en funksionering op enige stadium. journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η benadering tot verworwe disartrie 49 met die oog op die praktiese toepassing van die teoretiese besinning, is daar as volg te werk gegaan: 1. daar is 'n hierargiese spraakskema opgestel wat die neuro-evolusionere organisasie van spraakfisiologie aandui en wat as basis sal dien vir die opstel van 'n ondersoekprogram en riglyne sal verskaf vir die behandeling van verworwe disartrie. 2. 'n ondersoekprogram is saamgestel wat die spraakprobleem van die verworwe disartriegeval in sy totaliteit ondersoek en die neuro-evolusionere en neurofisiologiese simptome uitlig. bespreking van die hierargiese skema bekende feite in verband met spraak word hipoteties georden deur die skryfster en weergegee in die hierargiese skema. enkele vereistes is gestel aan die skema: 1. in ooreenstemming met neuro-evolusionere beginsels moet die skema spraakmotoriek voorstel vanaf die mees eenvoudige tot die meer komplekse, die mees outomatiese tot die meer willekeurige, en vanaf die mees georganiseerde tot die minder georganiseerde vlakke van funksionering. 2. die subsisteme van spraakfisiologie, nl. respirasie, fonasie, artikulasie, resonansie en vegetatiewe gedrag, word alles in ag geneem en ook die interskakeling moet na vore kom aangesien geeneen van die subsisteme onafhanklik bestaan nie. 3. die skakeling met sensoriese terugvoering word ook op die skema aangegee aangesien spraak sensories-motoriese gedrag is. 4. vir praktiese doeleindes moet die skema van waarde wees in die diagnose van motoriese spraakprobleme en ook riglyne verskaf vir die behandeling van die probleme. die skema word dan ook in vlakke verdeel wat stadia in die ontwikkeling van spraak en organisasie in die gestabiliseerde stadium aandui ten einde die teorie meer konkreet voor te stel. dit gee noodwendig aanleiding tot 'n mate van oorvereenvoudiging aangesien spraak in sy totaliteit uiters kompleks is en moeilik grafies voorgestel kan word. die voorstelling van die hierargiese spraakskema is soos reeds gese, bloot hipoteties en kan slegs deur verdere uitgebreide navorsing bevestig word. in 'n voorlopige ondersoek gedoen deur die skryfster, is bevind dat dit reeds in die huidige vorm tot 'n groot mate ooreenstem met die orde van devolusie en herevolusie in die verworwe disartriegeval en van groot praktiese waarde is. enkele aspekte van die skema word vervolgens bespreek: respirasie respirasie is die mees basiese funksie van alle lewende wesens en dit word slegs sekonder aangepas vir spraakdoeleindes deur geleidelike dieper respirasie met vinnige inspirasie en gekontroleerde verlengde ekspirasie. die uitvloei van lug word ook gevarieer om verhoogde subglottale druk te verkry wat aangepas word vir die klempatrone van 'n taal. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 anita van der merwe • 1> bfi.j. ̂ έ ΐ ϊ β β λ ό > — 4) μ +c bfi «• <υ.; £ έ 8 8 g > £ c cw 5·° ο g ο λ ό b w ο α> ^ τ3 c 60— ο c ο α> ή u. ο ο (λ c si'5 £ c ω « > α ω (λ — « ο ; « ο ® c " ζ sjs 3 •ξϋ-s a> 3 "i c t. 4> ̂ a) 4> ω — 4> tm c c tui 0 ο b co a. η <υ ο ε μ g.e ο ω •ο ο 5 ο α § ι ^ « j5 c c 3 « • he _ π ο c < 'e?s° ο ji" u sti· c π™ 5 ξ ο £ ο.» g " 3 ο"?-ξ 2.5 ο _ (λ 5 2 c ji ο ®·" 2 -a-src '•3 "ξ·!·* _ u π α μ >.2 ο c 2 u o n · ; i s i i ε » £ •.spas c > 52·α c w ω ρ ωΐ bo α> λ _ ~ « > 2 3 xj c d c _ «•"«•st .2 β c u « ο β> > vi .5 bo'w s 0) ι. ό a ι, μ · • c ; s-ji» « κ > i •5 ε c <υ a> a> q. t. "o •5os5 s = 3 1 3 t ζ ι c r " a ® ό c-^·0 c β ° >. π > c « μ c ε •ξ »c α» β> λ e v) ~ — c 5 sjc ta <λ·c 2 υ w m mm η —s u ο = 2 > μ 6 0 mt 5 si ο ̂ ·γ· * * 5 > : £ c _ w ω 3 a> 'vj "s 03 _ a> a> c ^ 3 « υ ο di a> a> (λ 2 o.c & λ 2 2 ^ π j2 s? o-: ο : c c oi ο /fcu " 2 journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η benadering tot verworwe disartrie t 51 fonasie glottale adduksie is aanvanklik suiwer refleksief en so ook die aanvanklike skakeling tussen ekspirasie en glottale sluiting tydens huil. vanwee die versterking deur sensoriese terugvoering, word die skakeling meer willekeurig en word glottale sluiting gebruik vir fonasie. die vaardigheid waarmee die fonasiemeganisme gebruik word en die diadokokinese van glottale abduksie en adduksie neem geleidelik toe, asook die vermoe om variasie in intonasie te produseer. die stippellyne dui op die begin van interskakeling wat op 'n latere stadium meer vasgele is, soos aangedui deur die vaste lyn. klempatrone en intonasievariasie in die bree konteks van spontane kommunikasie in enige taal word beskou as die mees komplekse vlak van interskakeling tussen fonasie en respirasie. vanuit 'n bewussynsvlak gesien kan die aspek nie op dieselfde vlak gestel word as artikulasie nie, want daar is beperkings op die willekeurige beheer van die aspekte vanwee die beperkte hoeveelheid proprioseptiewe inligting vanaf die fonasie en respirasie-meganisme. artikulasie die soekrefieks word gesien as die basis van artikulasiebeweging aangesien massabewegings van al die artikulasie organe en die kop hierin voorkom. dit is die beginpunt van latere onafhanklike bewegings. die liprefleks word ook hier geplaas hoewel dit ook by die meer suiwer vegetatiewe reflekse geplaas kan word. gei'soleerde lipronding is egter op latere stadiums waarskynlik meer verwant aan artikulasie as aan die basiese funksies. vanuit die soekrefieks ontwikkel kopkontrole en onafhanklike spraakorgaanbewegings. die mondrefleks wat 'n gekondisioneerde refleks is, ontstaan op 'n latere stadium en kan moontlik ook by die vegetatiewe funksies geplaas word, aangesien mondopening 'n voorvereiste is vir eet. dit kan egter ook gesien word as die basis vir latere willekeurige mondopening tydens artikulasie. terugvoering speel weer eens 'n belangrike rol in die verdere motoriese ontwikkeling en word op die skema aangedui. vaardigheid en diadokokinese neem geleidelik toe en artikulasie vereis akkurate plasing in steeds groter wordende kontekse namate die kind vorder vanaf woorde na sinne in sy taalontwikkeling. resonansie die velum dien aanvanklik as deflektor vir voedsel en hieruit kan afgelei word dat daar aanvanklik nie velere bewegings voorkom nie. namate die meer volwasse slukpatroon aangeneem word, is dit nodig dat 'n velere sluiting gevorm word. die beweging ontstaan waarskynlik dan op 'n latere stadium en word om die rede op vlak 2 aangedui. die sluiting word geleidelik gekoordineer met fonasie, die vaardigheid en diadokokinese neem toe, en nasalering vind plaas volgens die vereistes van die taal. vegetatiewe gedrag die aanvanklike infantiele orale reflekse word aangedui as die beginpunt van vegetatiewe gedrag. refieksiewe suig is die beginpunt van die volwasse slukrefieks. die slukrefleks soos byvoorbeeld vir speekselbeheer, word weer beskou as onderliggend aan die slukpatroon tydens eet in die volwasse stadium. retydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde. vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 anita van der merwe fleksiewe kou en byt vorm die basis vir latere gekoordineerde byt en kou. die suigfunksie word ook op vlak 3 aangedui aangesien dit 'n hoe mate van ko5rdinasie vereis tussen verskillende organe soos die velum, lippe en tong en slegs op 'n latere stadium moontlik is. vegetatiewe gedrag is die gebruik van die orale organe vir laer funksies wat nie-simbolies is nie. dit word egter nie streng gesproke gesien as die basis van artikulasie nie. die neurofisiologiese organisasie is waarskynlik totaal verskillend aangesien die vegetatiewe funksies onafhanklike kortikale verteenwoordiging het en dus nie net 'n laer orde organisasie van spraakbewegings is nie. om die rede word vegetatiewe gedrag as 'n onafhanklike subsisteem aangegee wat ook soos die ander subsisteme nou skakel met artikulasie aangesien dit steeds bewegings van dieselfde organe vereis. vereistes gestel aan die ondersoekprogram 1. afleidings in verband met neuro-evolusionere en neurofisiologiese aspekte van die probleem moet gemaak kan word met die oog op diagnose, behandeling en verdere navorsing. 2. die inligting verkry uit die program moet 'n indikasie gee van die algemene toestand en aard van die probleem aangesien die spraakprobleem as deel of simptoom van die totale probleem gesien moet word. 3. daar word nie gepoog om die probleem te klassifiseer nie, maar wel om die simptome van die toestand soos dit presenteer in elke individuele pasient, na te gaan. die literatuur dui aan dat die huidige klassifikasie van die motoriese sindroomgroepe nie onomwonde bewys en aanvaar word nie, en die neiging om elke persoon se individuele simptome na te gaan, word bespeur. 'n individuele benadering is aanvaarbaar en noodsaaklik as in ag geneem word dat die lokalisasie en dus ook die effek van 'n letsel verskil van persoon tot persoon. dit is egter so dat algemene beginsels met betrekking tot fisiologie ooreenstem in alle persone en aan die hand .van die beginsels kan alle persone wel ondersoek en behandel word. 4. elke subsisteem van spraak word nagegaan vanaf die mees eenvoudige tot die meer komplekse funksies ten einde 'n beeld van die totale spraakprobleem te verkry. 5. die aandeel van organiese bou in die funksionele probleem moet nie geignoreer word nie. die wyse waarop die probleem organies presenteer; byvoorbeeld in 'n verlamming, word ook aangeteken vanwee die belangrike rol in die spraakprobleem en die diagnostiese waarde daarvan. '' 6. ten einde vaste norms daar te stel, word die moontlike vorme wat 'n toestand kan aanneem aangegee, en die ondersoeke dui slegs met 'n kruisie die aard van 'n toestand aan. die metode is bok tydsbesparend. 7. die implikasies van die bevindings moet'ook aan die terapeut wat nie oor 'n diepgaande kennis van die neuro-evolusie beskik nie, riglyne verskaf vir die doelstellings van terapie. ; i 8. ruimte word egter ook gelaat vir kwalitatiewe beskrywings aangesien elke journal of the south a frican speech and hearing association, vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η benadering tot verworwe disartrie 53 individuele probleem in sy totaliteit nie deur keusemoontlikhede aangedui kan word nie. 9. die program moet met gemak en vinnig prakties toegepas kan word. 10. ruimte word gelaat vir die optekening van resultate op 'n latere stadium, byvoorbeeld na 'n periode van terapie aangesien die verloop van die toestand daaruit afgelei kan word, asook die effek van behandeling, medies sowel as terapeuties. vir navorsingsdoeleindes kan afleidings ook in verband met her-evolusie gemaak word. die ondersoekprogram vir verworwe disartrie algemene fisiese ondersoek 1. spiertonus: normaal; parese; paralise; verlaag; verhoog; links; regs; bilateraal. 2. reflekse: normaal; verhoog; verswak. 3. abnormale reflekse: afwesig; teenwoordig. 4. balans: kopkontrole: goed, swak, geen; kan sit; kan staan;kan loop. 5. onwillekeurige bewegings: afwesig; teenwoordig. 6. sensasie: normaal; verlies. 7. kraniaalsenuweeuitvalle: i-xii; geen. 8. diagnose: 9. prognose: opmerkings: die algemene fisiese ondersoek moet verkieslik deur die geneesheer voltooi word. perifere orale ondersoek die lippe en gesig, mandibula, tong en velum word aan die hand van die volgende aspekte ondersoek waar toepaslik. 1. bou: normaal; gebreke anatomies; atrofie. 2. spiertonus: normaal; verhoog; verlaag; links; regs; bilateraal. 3. simmetrie: normaal; asimmetries: trek na links; regs. 4. refleksiewe beweging: normaal; verhoog; verswak. 5. willekeurige beweging: normaal; swak; immobiel; feitlik immobiel. spraakmotoriek-ondersoek a. infantiele orale reflekse. 1. soekrefieks: teenwoordig; gedeeltelik; afwesig. 2. hand-mandibulere refleks: ditto. 3. liprefleks: ditto. 4. suigrefleks: ditto. 5. kourefleks: ditto. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23 desemer 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 anita van der merwe 6. bytrefleks: ditto. 7. mondoopmaakrefleks: ditto. 8. mandibulere strekrefleks. b. behoue reflekse. 1. slukrefleks: teenwoordig; verswak; verhoog; afwesig; vertraag. 2. speekselbeheer: ditto. 3. faringale refleks: ditto. 4. palatale refleks: ditto. 5. mandibulere refleks: ditto. 6. gaap: ditto. c. vegetatiewe funksies. 1. byt: normaal; verswak; afwesig; geassosieerde bewegings. 2. kou: ditto. 3. gesinkroniseerde byt, kou en sluk: ditto. 4. sluk: ditto. d. respirasie. 1. dominante tipe: abdominaal; torakaal; gemeng. 2. diepte van respirasie: normaal; vlak. 3. gebruik tydens rus: ekonomies; onekonomies. 4. reelmatigheid: egalig; onwillekeurige bewegings. 5. as aktivator vir refleksiewe glottale sluiting: voldoende; onvoldoende. 6. as aktivator vir fonasie: voldoende; onvoldoende. 7. duur van lang ekspirasie sonder stem: . . . sekondes. 8. inspirasiespoed vir spraak: normaal; stadig. 9. gebruik tydens spraak: ekonomies; onekonomies. 10. spraakluidheid: normaal; verswak; verhoog. 11. konstantheid van toonluidheid: normaal; neem geleidelik af; onwillekeurige bewegings. 12. ekspirasie vir klempatrone in woorde: voldoende; onvoldoende. 13. ekspirasie vir klem in groter konteks: voldoende; onvoldoende. e. fonasie. 1. glottale sluitrefleks, byvoorbeeld in hoes: teenwoordig; verswak; afwesig. 2. willekeurige koordinering met ekspirasie: voldoende; onmoontlik. 3. duur van verlengde fonasie: . . . sekondes. 4. konstantheid van verlengde fonasie: aanhoudend; wissel; neem vinnig af; onwillekeurige bewegings. journalof the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η benadering tot verworwe disartrie 55 5. inisieringspoed van stem: normaal; vertraag; gering vertraag. 6. diadokokinese van glottale sluiting: . . . keer in 5 sek., aaneenlopend. 7. ritme van glottale diadokokinese: ritmies normaal; stadig ritmies; a-ritmies 8. beweging tydens diadokokinese: afsonderlik; word aaneenlopend. 9. algemene toonhoogte: normaal; laag; hoog; wissel. 10. stemkwaliteit: normaal; swak. 11. toonhoogtevariasie in sinne: normaal; gering; monotoon. 12. toonhoogtevariasie in groter konteks: normaal; gering; monotoon. f. resonansie 1. palatale refleks: teenwoordig; afwesig; verswak; ontoetsbaar weens verhoogde faringale refleks. 2. koordinering met fonasie: teenwoordig, afwesig. 3. verlengde sametrekking met fonasie: normaal; vinnige uitputting; afwesig; onwillekeurige bewegings. 4. diadokokinese van velere sluiting: . . . keer in 5 sekondes. 5. kwaliteit tydens diadokokinese: goed; onvoldoende bewegings; bewegings worcl aaneenlopend. 6. nasale resonansie in spraak: normaal; hipernasaal; hiponasaal. 7. graad van nasaliteit: gering; erg; totaal. g. artikulasie. spraaksisteemdifferensiasie: goed; gedeeltelik; geen. 1. kop van toraks. 2. artikulators van kop en nek. 3. lippe van kaak en tong. 4. tong van kaak en lippe. 5. kaak van tong en lippe. 6. intra-orgaandifferensiasie. diadokokinese: . . . keer in 5 sek.; ontoetsbaar. 1. enkelartikulasie. i) nie-spraakbewegings: tong in en uit; tong heen en weer; lippe; kaak. ii) spraakbewegings: tong; lippe; kaak; velum (bv. la; pa; ka; pm) 2. twee-plekartikulasie. i) voor na agter in mond. ii) voor na mid del. iii) agter na voor. iv) agter na middel. 3. drie-plekartikulasie i) voor, middel en agter van mond. ii) agter, middel en voor. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 anita van der merwe iii) middel, voor en agter. artikulasie 1. aard van artikulasie: akkuraat; goed; swak; baie swak; uiters swak; weggelaat. i) bilabiaal. ii) labiodentaal. iii) palataal. iv) alveoler. v) veler. vi) vokale. vii) klankverbindings. 2. algemene indruk van artikulasie: goeie artikulasie; verswakte artikulasie; uiters verswak; erg verswak; weinig artikulasiebewegings. 3. gelykvormigheid van artikulasie in spontane spraak: konstant dieselfde geartikuleer; varieer; breuke in artikulasie; verswak met uitputting. artikulasie tydens diadokokinese: 1. kontinui'teit van bewegings: bly behoue; neem vinnig af; geleidelik; onwillekeurige bewegings; feitlik immobiel. 2. ritme tydens diadokokinese toetsing: ritmies; stadig ritmies; a-ritmies. 3. akkuraatheid van plasing: goed; swak; baie swak. 4. geassosieerde bewegings van: ander artikulators; ander liggaamsbewegings. 5. spoed van spontane spraak: normaal; stadig; baie stadig; onreelmatig. h. vlotheid van spraak: 1. vlotheid van spraak: normaal; onvlot. 2. ritmespoed van spraak: normaal; stadig; wissel. i. kompensasie. kompenseer deur: geen; spoed van spraak; addisionele bewegings. j. verstaanbaarheid. 1. verstaanbaarheid van spontane spraak: goed; swak; baie swak. 2. verstaanbaarheid tydens: woorde; sinne; spontane spraak. k. indruk van algemene erns van toestand: uiters swak; baie.swak; swak; gemiddeld; gering; weinig. /* behandeling van verworwe disartrie nadat die vlak van funksionering van elke subsisteem van spraak bepaal is met behulp van die ondersoekprogram, is die doel van terapie om waar moontlik, 'n herevolusie te bewerkstellig. in 'h degeneratiewe of statiese toestand word ondersteuningsterapie gedoen om devolusie tee te werk. binne die raamwerk wat die skema bied kan verskeie tegnieke toegepas word ten einde die neurofisiologiese disfunksie, byvoorbeeld 'n parese, te verbeter. keuse van tegnieke journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 23 december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η benadering tot verworwe disartrie 57 word aan die diskresie van die terapeut oorgelaat ten einde aan te pas by die behoeftes van die pasient. die metode wat gevolg moet word kom neer op: 1. lnhibisie van positiewe simptome indien dit voorkom. 2. fasilitasie van negatiewe simptome. die metode word hoofsaaklik gevolg in spraakterapie aangesien spraak as 'n hoer funksie hoofsaaklik uit negatiewe simptome bestaan. tegnieke wat gebruik kan word, is die volgende: 1. mysak11' 1 2 ' 1 3 ,beskryf tegnieke wat veral waardevol op die refleksvlakke gebruik kan word. 2. proprioseptiewe neuromuskulere fasilitasietegnieke wat gerig is op die verbetering van onderliggende bewegingspatrone, is uiters geslaagd. die tegnieke is veral van waarde by fasilitering van die middelvlakke, byvoorbeeld verlengde ekspirasie en onafhanklike orgaanbewegings. 3. willekeurige pogings, byvoorbeeld by diadokokinese verbetering. 4. tradisionele spraakterapeutiese grondbeginsels, byvoorbeeld multisensoriese stimulasie en motivering van pasient. hippocrates het gese dat genesing nie slegs afhanklik is van tyd nie, maar ook van geleentheid, en dit is die verantwoordelikheid van die spraakterapeut om aan elke pasient wat in haar sorg geplaas word, die maksimum geleentheid tot herstel te bied. verwysings 1. bobath, k. (1969): the motor deficit in patients with cerebral palsy. clinics in developmental medicine no. 23. william heineman medical books. 2. brain, r. (1956): diseases of the nervous system. university press, london. 3. crickmay, m.c. (1972): speech therapy and the bobath approach to cerebral palsy. charles c. thomas, illinois. 4. crouch, z.b. (1971): lip taping for buccal-labial insufficiency. /. speech hear. dis., 36, 543-545. 5. darley, f.l., aronson, a.e. & brown, j.r. (1969): differential diagnostic patterns in dysarthria. / speech hear. res., 12, 246-270. 6. darley, f.l., aronson, a.e. & brown, j.r. (1969): clusters of deviant speech dimensions in the dysarthrias. /. speech hear. res., 12, 462-297. 7. hixon, t.j. & hardy, j.c. (1964): restricted motility of the speech articulators in cerebral palsy. /. speech hear. dis., 29, 293-306. 8. ingram, t.t.s. (1973): developmental disorders of speech. handbook of clinical neurology, vol. 4, disorders of speech, perception and symbolic behaviour. american elsevier publishing co., new york. 9. jackson, j.h. (1925): neurological fragments. oxford medical publications. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse, vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23, desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 anita van der merwe 10. morley, d.e. (1955): the rehabilitation of adults with dysarthric speech. j. speech hear. dis., 20, 58-64. 11. mysak, e.d. (1959): significance of neuro-physiological orientation to cerebral palsy habilitation. j. speech hear. dis., 24, 221-230. 12. mysak, e.d. (1963): dysarthria and oropharyngeal reflexology: a review. j. speech hear. dis., 28, 252-260. 13. mysak, e.d. (1968): ν euro-evolutional approach to cerebral palsy and speech. teachers college press, new york. 14. peacher, w.c. (1950): the etiology and differential diagnosis of dysarthria./. speech hear. dis., 15, 252-265. 15. tikofsky, r.s. & tikofsky, r.p. (1964): intelligibility measures of dysarthric speech. / speech hear. res., 7, 325-333. 16. wedin, s. (1972): rehabilitation of speech in cases of palato-pharyngeal paresis with the aid of an obturator prosthesis. british j. dis., comm., 7, 117-129. 17. weiss, d.a. & beebe, h.h. (1951): the chewing approach in speech and voice therapy. s. karger, new york. 18. west, r.w., ansberry, m. & carr, c. (1957): the rehabilitation of speech. harper & brothers, new york. 19. wolski, w. (1967): hypernasality as the presenting symptom of myasthenia gravis. /. speech hear. dis., 32, 36-38. 20. wyke, b. (1966): recent advances in the neurology of phonation: phonatory reflex mechanisms in the larynx. speech pathology diagnosis: theory and practice, report of the national conference of the college of speech therapists in glasgow. e. & s. livingstone, london. 21. wyke, b. (1970): neurological mechanisms in stammering: an hypothesis. british j. dis., comm., 5, 6-15. journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 23. december 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) tonal purity is our speaaiite de la maison . . . and it has been since 1948, when we began. they tell us that today's state-of-the-art of audioelectronics owes much to studer research and specialisation in this field. . . . we're human. so we blush. but we must agree that we design and manufacture audio equipment of superb quality. in a product range unequalled in its bracket . . . which is the highest. we supply this equipment to professionals and discerning amateurs throughout the world. they buy it for its tonal purity. . . . and that, as we said, is the speciality of our house. studer · revox · akg · thorens professional recording and audio mixing equipment for radio. television, motion-picture and disc-recording studios studer-revox south africa (pty)ltd microphones. amplifiers, turntables, fm-tuners, loudspeaker systems, language laboratory installations, audiovisual teaching equipment toiai^-h^ubx s t u d e r r e v o x s o u t h a f r i c a (ptyl ltd., audward house. 3 0 ameshoff street, braamfontein. 2001. tel. 724-9680/88 .p.o. box 3 1 2 8 2 , braamfontein. 2017. telex 8-6683 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 23. desember 1976 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a speech production changes with the use of a multichannel cochlear implant in a postlingually hearing impaired adult sandy cummings emily groenewald rene hugo department of communication pathology university of pretoria lida miiller department of speech pathology and audiology university of stellenbosch mike van der linde department of information technology division of academic computing university of pretoria abstract profoundly deaf cochlear implant users provide an interesting population in which to assess the role of distorted auditory feedback in speech, since their electrically stimulated hearing is significantly different from normal hearing. the aim of the study was to evaluate, by means of spectrograph^ and listener analyses, the speech production changes in a postlingually deafened adult with the use of a multichannel cochlear implant over time, compared to that of hearing aids as well as no-amplification. the results indicated significant improvements in the use of suprasegmental speech features as well in the production of specific segmental features of speech. opsomming uitermatige dowe pasiente met kogleere inplantings vorm 'n interessante populasie waarby die invloed van versteurde ouditiewe terugvoer op spraak ondersoek kan word, aangesien elektries-gestimuleerde gehoor betekenisvol verskil van normale gehoor. die doel van die studie was 'n ondersoek na die invloed van 'n multikanaal kogleere inplanting oor 'n verloop van tyd, in vergelyking met geen ouditiewe versterking en versterking dmv gehoorapparate op spraakproduksie van 'n postlinguale dowe volwassene. 'n vergelykende spektrografiese en luisteraaranalise is uitgevoer. die resultate het betekenisvolle verbeteringe getoon in die suprasegmentele, sowel as spesifieke segmentele eienskappe van spraak. introduction research on the benefits of cochlear implants have in the past focused primarily on the speech perception of the postlingually deafened implant user (dowell, brown, seligman & clark, 1985; dowell, seligman, blarney & clark, 1987; eddington, 1983; cohen, waltzman & shapiro, 1985 and schindler, kessler, rebscher, yanda & jackler, 1986). improvements in environmental sound recognition as well as improvements in auditory sensitivity (thielemeir, eisenberg & brimacombe, 1982; tyler, gantz, mccabe, lowder, otto & preece, 1985) have also been widely documented. an increasing number of researchers have begun to place emphasis on the speech production characteristics of postlingually deafened implant wearers, (iler-kirk & edgerton, 1983; leder, spitzer, milner, flevarisphillips, richardson & kirchner, 1986; tartter, chute & hellman, 1989; waldstein, 1990; lane & webster, 1991; economou, tartter, chute & hellman, 1992; perkell, lane, svirsky & webster, 1992; svirsky, lane, perkell & wosniak, 1992). the lack of research directed at the speech production changes could be due to the fact that a postlingually acquired deafness does not necessarily lead to problems of speech degeneration (ling, 1976). studies designed to investigate the speech die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 sandy cummings, emily groenewald, rene hugo, lida miiller, mike van der linde characteristics of postlingually deaf subjects have, however, found that longterm total auditory deprivation in hearing adults resulted in eventual deterioration into flat, unmodulated and dysprosodic voice, (cowie, douglas-cowie & kerr, 1982). waldstein (1990) investigated some effects of postlingual deafness on speech by exploring selected properties of consonants, vowels and s u p r a s e g m e n t a l s in the speech of seven totally, postlingually deafened individuals. their results demonstrated that postlingual deafness affects the production of all the classes of speech sounds. re-introduction of "auditory input" with the electrical stimulation of the auditory nerve should result in changes in speech production. these changes in speech production are primarily reliant on the type of information derived from stimulation and finally the implant listener's articulatory adaptation to prolonged profound deafness, (perkell et al., 1992). the most common changes in speech production with the use of multichannel cochlear implants can be described in terms of suprasegmental and segmental changes, although previous studies have indicated that the suprasegmental features are affected the most due to the presence of a profound hearing loss, (leder et al., 1986). waldstein, (1990) indicated that the results of his study do not support the hypothesis that the suprasegmental properties of speech show the greatest effects of a loss of auditory feedback, while the segmental properties remain relatively unaffected. "rather, auditory feedback appears to be implicated in monitoring and maintaining phonetic precision in all classes of speech sounds." waldstein, (1990:211). various s t u d i e s i n v e s t i g a t i n g the effects of multichannel cochlear implants on speech production have indicated that significant changes in speech production can occur with the use of a multichannel implant. lane & webster, (1991) have reviewed some of the studies investigating the speech of postlingually deafened speakers including several studies on the effects of cochlear implants. they conclude that the majority of studies implicate a role for audition in regulating, directly or indirectly, several speech properties including voice quality, voice aspiration, vocal duration, pitch, intonation, stress, tempo, nasality and frication and plosive articulation, (iler-kirk & edgerton 1983; leder et al., 1986; tartter et al., 1989; economou et al., 1992; perkell et al., 1992). method aims the aim of the present study was to evaluate the speech production changes brought about by the use of a multichannel cochlear implant over time, in an adult with a profound postlingual sensorineural hearing loss. the subject's speech production skills were evaluated preand post-operatively by means of spectrographic and listener analyses. these evaluations included: the spectrographic evaluation of the suprasegmental features of speech, in terms of sentence duration, fundamental frequency variations and word stress (amplification). the spectrographic evaluation of the segmental features of speech, namely vowels and consonants. the vowels were evaluated in terms of vowel length and the formant relationships between f1 and f2. the consonants were evaluated in terms of the spectral noise band frequencies for fricatives, the spectral frequency ranges for the plosive sections of the stop sounds and the relative amplitude peaks of both the fricatives and plosives. the listener's analysis of speech production was evaluated in terms of pitch variation, and vocal and pausal duration. the results of the speech production changes with the multichannel cochlear implant over time were compared to the subject's speech production without any amplification and with binaural hearing aids. research methodology a single-case research study using a time series experimental design was used in order to evaluate the speech p r o d u c t i o n changes with the use of a multichannel cochlear implant in an adult with a postlingual sensorineural hearing loss. the data was obtained by performing evaluations in the following phases: pre-implant: no-amplification (na); hearing aid (ha) post-implant: 0-months (ci-0), 3-months (ci.3) and 6-months (ci-6) cochlear implant subject the subject (mc) lost his hearing due to meningitis at 19 years of age. he was implanted five years later. his pre-implant audiograms indicated a bilaterally total sensorineural hearing loss with minimal benefit from binaural hearing aids as well as a vibrotactile device. his right ear was implanted on 31 october 1991 with the cochlear 22-channel implant. all electrodes were easily inserted. the electrodes were programmed in the bipolar + 1 mode using the mpeak coding strategy. the subject (mc) was an afrikaans speaker using a nonstandard dialect. implant and speech processor t the cochlear implant used in the present ^tudy is known as the mini-system 22 comprising 22 pure platinum bands supported on a flexible silicone rubber carrier. the speech processor utilized is referred to as the msp (mini speech processor). the mpeak coding strategy extracts and codes f1 and f2, where the f1 is represented by the dominant spectral peak in the range from approximately 300-1000hz and f2 between 8004000hz. the estimate of f1 from the acoustic signal controls the selection of an apical electrode pair, while the estimate of f2 controls the selection of a basal electrode pair. the spectral energy in the regions of 20002800hz, 2800-4000hz and above 4000hz are presented to three basally located electrodes. the fundamental or voicing frequency determines the pulse rate of the stimulation and the amplitude of the acoustic signal determines the stimulus intensity, (cochlear, 1989). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech production changes with the use of a multichannel cochlear implant in a postlingually hearing impaired adult hearing aids the two phonak ppc-l-4 hearing aids were utilized in the pre-implant evaluation condition. the maximum output of the hearing aids is 142 db sspl with a frequency range of 140 4900 hz. the subject used the hearing aid on volume 2,5 with the following settings: lc 1 , hc 5, g 6 and sspl 6. the subject also wore new standard acrylic ear moulds. stimuli and equipment the data for the evaluation of the subject's speech production skills were determined objectively and subjectively by means of spectrographic and listener analyses respectively. for the spectrographic analysis of the suprasegmental features of speech, seven sentences varying in length and including the various typical prosodic features of statements, questions and commands, were selected to be spectrographically analyzed. for the spectrographic analysis of the segmental features, eight isolated single vowels; / i /, / a /, = / y /, / oe /, / u /, / a /, / ο / & / ε / in cvc combinations were selected to be analyzed. the vowels selected were the most common vowels utilized in afrikaans where the transition to diphtongs does not take place. the consonants selected were those consonants which are either ommitted or distorted in the presence of a profound hearing loss (nickerson, 1975). the plosives, / t / f / k / , / p / and the fricatives / s / and / χ / were used in the final position and / s / and / f / were used in the initial position of the word. these consonants were analyzed within the context of two different words and preceded or followed by different vowels. for the listener analysis, the subject was required to read a paragraph in which the length, language, content and complexity was appropriate for the purpose of the evaluation as well as the subject's reading skills. the specified material wfas read into a dynamic m 0 2 ncc microphone (15cm from the microphone) in an i ac soundproof and sound reverberation free unit. the recordings were made on a direct head casette deck. nachamitchi, model 682 2x the speaker's distance from the microphone as well as the input attenuation were kept c o n s t a n t t h r o u g h o u t all the r e c o r d i n g s . spectrographic analyses wrere conducted using the kay dsp sonagraph, model 5500. the analysed data was graphically displayed on ainec/multisync colour monitor. procedure recording of data recordings of all the stimulus words and sentences were made without any amplification of the signal in the pre-implant condition, with binaural hearing aids in the pre-implant condition, the following day after switch-on i.e., 0months; 3-months and 6-months postimplant. spectrographic analysis for the analysis of the suprasegmental features, a combination analysis setup of the sonagraph, whereby the waveform, amplitude and pitch trace of the sentences were displayed, was used for the measurements. the sentence duration was measured by applying time cursors on the speech wave display. pitch variation was determined by using the frequency cursor readings on the computed pitch curves at the highest and lowest peaks in the pitch trace. word stress or amplification was measured by the time cursors at the highest peak of computed amplitude curves. the analysis of segmental features of specific speech sounds focused on the duration and formant frequencies of vowels and the spectral characteristics of the fricatives and the plosive energy of stop consonants. the vowels were analyzed up to 4000hz and the consonants up to 8000hz. a combination display of a wideband spectrogram in conjunction with a narrowband amplitude spectrum of the computed spectral energy between two time cursors was used. time cursors were used in order to determine the length of the vowel. for the measurements of the formant frequencies, a stable middle portion of each vowel was selected in order to exclude the transitions to the consonants. frequency cursors, in conjunction with the time cursors were utilized in order to determine the formant frequencies. the consonants were analyzed in terms of the spectral noise band frequencies for the fricatives (i.e., minimum and maximum frequencies of the fricatives), the spectral frequency range for the plosive sections of the stop sound (i.e., minimum and maximum frequencies of the plosive sections of the stop sounds) and the relative amplitude peaks of the fricatives and plosives. time and frequency cursors were used to determine the frequency boundaries as well as the relative amplitude peaks. listener analysis for the listener's evaluation of the subject's speech production, a rating scale for listener's evaluation of pitch variation and vocal and pausal duration was used, (coles, 1990). the recordings were presented to four trained listeners via tandberg educational headphones in a language laboratory by means of a tandberg educational is 10 cassette player. the listeners were asked to evaluate pitch variation, vocal duration and pausal duration by means of a five point rating scale (appendix a). results results of the speech production evaluation using spectrographic analysis. it is important to note that there were no marked abnormal characteristics pertaining to the subject's suprasegmental and segmental features as found in some postlingually deafened adults. spectrographic analyses were executed in order to detect any degeneration of speech features which had possibly occurred during the period of profound deafness as well as any changes in speech production brought about by the use of the cochlear implant. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 sandy cummings, emily groenewald, rene hugo, lida m l l e r , mike van der linde suprasegmental features the results of sentence duration across various auditory input situations are presented in table 1. when analyzed, the majority of the sentences showed an increase in sentence duration at the six month cochlear implant level, as opposed to the no-amplification and hearing aid conditions. the increases in sentence duration occurred at either the cochlear implant 0-month or 3-month situation, with a highest mean duration for the seven sentences at 0-months. a qualitative analysis of the individual sentences does, however, give logical explanations for these results. the interpretation and explanation of the above findings will be given in the discussion of results. as can be seen in table 2, the highest relative amplitude peaks for stressed words showed a small, however, consistent increase in the amplitude range with the use of the cochlear implant, with the values ranging from 33,ldb, 37,4db and 37db during the 0-, 3and 6-month cochlear implant intervals, compared to 31,7db and 34db obtained during the no-amplification and hearing aid conditions respectively. the values for all.sentences obtained during the 3-month interval showed an increase in amplitude peaks when compared to the 0month interval. during the 6-month interval, however, there was a slight decrease in amplitude peaks for sentences two, four and six when compared to the 3-month cochlear implant interval. these values compared to the 0-month interval did, however, show an increase in amplitude range. the results of the fundamental frequency (f0) variations, including the minimum and maximum f0 values as well as the differences in f0 across the various auditory input situations, are presented in table 3. as can be seen in table 3, the mean difference in fundamental frequency variations for the no-amplification (49,7hz) and hearing aid (48,7hz) situations did not differ significantly from one another. surprisingly, the results for the 0-month cochlear implant interval showed a decrease in f0 variation (44,4hz). a significant increase in the mean f0 variation occurred at the 3-month interval (110,7hz), indicating a possible over compensation in the use of varying f0. the results obtained at the 6-month interval (86,5hz) also showed an increase in f0 variation when compared to the no-amplification and hearing aid situations, however, the variation was less and possibly more normalized, than observed during the 3-month interval. segmental features vowels due to variations in vowel duration for each of the vowels within the three cochlear implant evaluation intervals, a mean vowel duration for each of the eight vowels during the cochlear implant intervals was calculated. the results of the evaluation of vowel duration presented in table 4, did not indicate significant table 1. sentence duration (seconds) for sentences across various auditory input situations. sentence number n.a. h.a. ci-0 ci-3 ci-6 mean values ci 0, 3, 6 1 0,8719 0,9750 0,9937 0,9031 0,8844 0,927 2 1,153 1,272 1,438 1,472 1,387 1,432 3 1,700 1,727 2,425 1,993 1,878 2,098 4 1,537 1,691 1,628 1,600 1,547 1,591 5 1,237 1,575 1,456 1,359 1,322 1,379 6 0,9469 0,9688 0,9562 1,031 0,9312 0,972 7 0,8031 0,9600 1,006 0,9750 0,9125 0,964 table 2. highest amplitude peaks (db) for stressed words in sentences across various auditory input sentence number n.a h.a ci-0 ci-3 ci-6 1 25 35 33 37 37 2 24 32 30 37 35 3 37 34 31 36 37 4 34 30 33 36 35 5 34 34 37 40 41 6 33 35 32 39 37 7 35 38 36 37 37 mean amplitude peaks (db) 31,7 34 33,1 37,4 37,0 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e e c h production changes with the use of a multichannel cochlear implant in a postlingually hearing impaired adult differences between the various auditory input situations. for all the vowels examined except for the vowel /y/, however, when a mean value for the 0-, 3and 6m o n t h evaluations was calculated, the latter did show a decrease in vowel duration when compared to either the no-amplification or hearing aid condition. the formant frequencies of the vowels analyzed, i.e., f1 and f2, presented in table 5, did not differ from one another significantly across the various auditory input situations. compared to the normal values of f1 and f2 in the vowels of male afrikaans speakers, (van der merwe, groenewald, van aardt, tesner & grimbeek, 1993), the formant frequencies of four of the vowels i.e. / i /, / a /, = / u / and / ε / fell within the normal limits. in the four remaining vowels i.e. / oe /, / ο / and /oe/ examined except for / y /, the f2 values fell within the normal limits for these vowels. the f1 values, however, 7 were measured at higher frequencies than the normal limits for f l . the results in the present study did, however, indicate that the f l and f2 values did remain relatively consistent across the various auditory input situations as was observed with the other four vowels examined. consonants as can be seen in table 6 for the majority of the consonants examined in the present study, the use of the cochlear implant did show an increase in the relative amplitude peaks measured for consonant production i.e., plosives and fricatives. the relative amplitude peaks for the consonants in the various auditory input situations ranged from 0-46db in the no-amplification situation, 0-50db in the hearing aid situation, 19-5ldb in table 3. minimum and maximum fundamental frequency values (fo-hz) and difference in fundamental frequency (δ fo) for sentences across various auditory input situations. sentence number max fo min fo n.a n.a δ fo h.a h.a δ fo ci-0 ci-0 δ fo ci-3 ci-3 δ fo ci-6 ci-6 δ fo 1 max 162 45 157 70 134 34 243 137 222 103 1 min 117 45 87 70 100 34 106 137 119 103 2 max 132 43 129 45 124 38 204 113 182 80 2 min 89 43 84 45 86 38 91 113 102 80 3 max 170 ' 89 150 62 129 48 213 115 213 89 3 min 81 ' 89 88 62 81 48 98 115 124 89 4 max 144 28 128 30 138 35 182 56 186 69 4 min 116 28 98 30 103 35 126 56 117 69 5 max 179 73 165 72 186 89 262 176 256 149 5 min 106 73 93 72 97 89 86 176 107 149 6 max t 144 27 142 42 124 23 196 79 165 54 6 min 1 117 27 100 42 101 23 117 79 111 54 7 max 162 43 167 60 144 44 222 99 204 62 7 min 119 43 107 60 100 44 123 99 142 62 mean difference in fo ! 49,7 54,4 44,4 110,7 86,5 table 4. vowel duration (seconds) for vowels across various auditory input situations word vowel n.a. h.a. ci-0 ci-3 ci-6 mean mier /i / 0,4812 0,4016 0,4187 0,2469 0,3125 0,3260 waak /a/ 0,2187 0,3719 0,2813 0,3875 0,3562 0,3416 duur /y/ 0,3375 0,3156 0,4812 0,3312 0,4590 0,4238 dop /d/ 0,1469 0,1344 0,1500 0,1312 0,1094 0,1302 toer /u/ 0,5875 0,5594 0,4906 0,4094 0,4031 0,4343 pen /ε/ 0,1656 0,1563 0,1781 0,1187 0,1156 0,1374 rug /oe/ 0,1219 0,1656 0,1594 0,1281 0,1563 0,1479 mis /a/ 0,0781 0,1312 0,1344 0,0962 0,0734 0,1013 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 sandy cummings, emily groenewald, rene hugo, lida muller, mike van der linde table 5. formant frequencies hz (f/fj) for vowels across various auditory input situations word vowel f / f 2 n.a h.a ci-0 ci-3 ci-6 mean ci 0, 3, 6 normal f'/f '•• ·.. mier /i / f. 300 260 300 300 280 293 245 2180 2190 2160 2320 2380 2287 2186 waak (a) fi 760 620 770 640 600 670 679 f 2 1180 1160 1180 1220 1060 1153 1113 duur /y/ 260 240 220 390 290 300 * f 2 2180 2160 2080 2310 2470 2287 * dop ν fi 580 280 540 540 590 557 373 f 2 1020 1080 1180 1040 1040 1087 805 toer /u/ f , 260 250 260 390 360 337 266 f 2 780 920 780 800 1010 863 961 pen /ε/ f , 500 480 500 560 530 530 353 f 2 1960 2040 2060 2100 2000 2053 2055 rug /ce/ f, 640 540 560 580 580 573 429 f 2 1280 1200 1240 1160 1040 1147 1314 mis /a/ f, 580 540 540 540 600 560 507 f 2 1400 1520 1480 1470 1330 1427 1514 table 6: relative amplitude peaks (db) for consonants in cvc word combinations tory input situations word consonant position no amplification hearing aid ci 0 months ci 3 months ci 6 months lied /t/ final 0 38 23 35 48 pit /t/ final 29 18 31 47 j 48 loop /p/ final 0 19 22 23 ! 21 druip /p/ final 0 0 19 31 j 29 bek /k/ final 26 0 20 37 1 39 rok pej final 0 20 21 33 ' 36 kous /s/ final 31 43 48 44 48 mos /s/ final 44 43 48 44 49 rug /x/ final 33 43 27 38 36 saag /x/ final 29 35 34 33 31 sag /s/ initial 43 50 51 29 54 saag /s/ initial 46 48 48 54 52 vaak m initial 19 34 22 34 28 vuur m initial 33 34 26 29 32 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e e c h production changes with the use of a multichannel cochlear implant in a postlingually hearing impaired adult the 0-month cochlear implant interval, 23-54db in the 3 m o n t h interval and 21-54db in the 6-month interval. three of the consonant productions in the final position j e / 1 /, / k / and / ρ / were not produced with any audible'plosive release in the no-amplification and hearing aid situations therefore having no measurable amplitude peaks i.e., odb. the results of the mean spectral frequency ranges for the plosive energy of the plosives i.e., / 1 /, / ρ /, and / k / and the spectral noise band frequencies for the fricatives i.e., / s /, i x l and / f /, presented in table 7, did show a narrowing of these frequency ranges for the majority of consonants evaluated in the three cochlear implant intervals when compared to the hearing aid conditions. once again there were no measurable spectral frequency ranges (0hz) for three of the consonants i.e., / ρ /, / t / and / k /, as there was no audible plosive release in the final position during the no-amplification and hearing aid conditions. 9 results of the speech production evaluation using listener analysis. the results of the listener's evaluation of the subject's speech production are presented in table 8. the trained listeners rated the subject's spontaneous speech at the various auditory input situations, according to the rating scale used. these ratings occurred in a language laboratory. as can be seen in table 8 the listeners rated the subject's use of pitch variation as being monotone in the no-amplification, hearing aid and 0-month cochlear implant conditions. at the 3-month cochlear implant interval, the listeners rated the subject's speech as having little variation and the 6-month cochlear implant interval the listeners rated the subject's use of pitch variation as being normal. with regard to vocal duration, the listeners evaluated the subject's vocal duration in the no-amplification, hearing aid and cochlear implant 0-month interval as being longer than normal. table 7. minimum and maximum frequencies for consonants in cvc word combinations across various auditory input situations. word consonant position minimum no amp ha ci-0 ci-3 ci-6 maximum lied /t/ final min 0 1760 2460 1680 2880 max 0 8000* 4900 8000* 6040 pit 111 final min 3440 1760 2480 2720 3400 max 6800 3160 8000* 8000* 8000* loop /p/ final min 0 3560 1080 1160 3680 max 0 4720 8000* 7960 6440 druip /p/ final min 0 0 1040 1240 980 max 0 0 8000* 8000* 8000* bek pej final min 880 0 1060 960 880 max 8000* 0 5000 5600 6240 rok pej final min 0 960 880 680 640 max 0 4280 5020 8000* 3680 kous isl | final min 2420 2240 2500 2720 3200 max 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* mos isl ! final min 2220 2360 2480 2560 3160 max 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* rug ixl final min 800 760 2480 2640 2320 max 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* 6960 saag ixl final min 2460 880 2060 2760 2280 max 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* 7440 sag isl initial min 2180 3200 1080 2800 3320 1 max 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* saag isl initial min 1000 3320 1060 2960 3000 max 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* vaak ifl initial min 2100 3440 3480 1360 3680 max 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* vuur ifl initial min 1360 3520 1620 2880 3600 max 8000* 8000* 8000* 8000* 7360 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 sandy cummings, emily groenewald, ren hugo, lida m l l e r , mike van der linde at the 3-month and 6-month cochlear implant intervals, the subject's vocal duration was evaluated as being normal. as far as pausal duration is concerned, the listeners evaluated the use of pausal duration as being primarily longer than normal in the no-amplification and hearing aid conditions. at the cochlear implant 0-month interval, two of the listeners rated the pausal duration as being normal, with the remaining listeners describing it as being either abnormally long or longer than normal. the pausal duration at the 3-month interval was evaluated by the majority of the listeners as being longer than normal. one listener judged it to be normal. at the 6-month cochlear implant interval, however, the subject's use of pausal duration was evaluated as being normal. as can be seen in table 8, listener two was the only listener who differed from the other listeners. the pearson correlation co-efficient and t-test was used in order to determine whether or not listener two differed significantly from the other listeners, i.e., one, three and four. these results indicated that there were no significant differences between the listeners ratings and that all the listeners ratings correlated significantly with one another. discussion spectrographic analysis suprasegmental features prior to having the cochlear implant, it was noticed subjectively that the subject was inclined to increase his utterance length. the latter being a characteristic of profoundly deaf individuals, as noted by nickerson, (1975). taking this into account, it was expected that when utterance length was spectrographically analyzed, the results obtained with the cochlear implant would show a decrease in duration, when compared to the noamplification and hearing aid condition. results obtained from the evaluation of sentence duration (table 1), however, did not at first yield any significant data. in fact, the mean values calculated for the cochlear implant condition were in all instances longer than the values obtained in the no-amplification situation, as well as the majority of values obtained during the hearing aid condition. these results are in direct contrast to the sentence duration characteristics of profoundly deaf individuals. these individuals tend to speak at a slower rate than what is considered normal speed, (waldstein, 1990). the values measured were consequently compared with the spectrograms obtained for each of the sentences in the various auditory input situations. these comparisons indicated that the reason for the lower sentence duration values in the no-amplification situation and the higher sentence duration values in the cochlear implant situations was that in many instances the plosive release of final consonants of the last words spoken in the sentences were often omitted due to the absence of auditory feedback. the utterance length measured did therefore not include these omitted plosive portions of the consonants. this resulted in shorter sentence duration measurements. during the three cochlear implant intervals the subject was aware of these final consonants due to the improved speech information being provided by the cochlear implant as well as improved auditory feedback, and the subsequent production of these final consonants productions resulted in a measured increase in the utterance length. as far as word stress (amplification) within the sentences is concerned, the use of the cochlear implant resulted in an increase in the highest relative amplitude peaks for stressed words when compared to the no-amplification and hearing aid situations (table 2). the specific words within the sentences which were stressed remained consistent throughout all the auditory input situations. the relative amplitude peaks of these words increased as the subject was receiving increasing evaluation parameters listeners no-amplification: listeners ratings hearing aid: listener ratings cochlear implant 0-months: listener ratings cochlear implant 3-months: listener ratings cochlear implant 6 months: listener ratings pitch l.l 1 1 1 2 3 variation l.2 1 2 2 3 3 l.3 1 1 1 2 3 l.4 1 1 1 2 3 vocal l.l 2 2 2 3 "3 duration l.2 2 2 2 3 4 l.3 2 2 2 3 3 l.4 2 2 2 3 / 3 pausal l.l 2 3 3 2 3 duration l.2 1 2 2 2 3 l.3 2 2 1 2 3 l.4 2 2 3 '3 3 tabel 8. rating scale for listeners' evaluation of pitch variation, vocal duration and pausal durationresults . the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e e c h production changes with the use of a multichannel cochlear implant in a postlingually hearing impaired adult 11 a m o u n t s of intensity information via the cochlear imnlant. these alterations in loudness are coded by the multichannel cochlear implant as changes in current level, (tobey & hasenstab, 1991). an individual's ability to make use of intensity variations within utterances p r e v e n t s the tendency to become monotone and reinforces the normal patterns of intonation in speech, (ilerkirk & edgerton, 1983). in terms of the pitch variations in fo, there appeared to be an over compensation in the use of varying fundamental frequency at the 3-month interval, with a slight decrease in variation at the 6-month interval, resulting in a more normalized version of fundamental frequency v a r i a t i o n s . the subject was, therefore, at the 3-month interval beginning to perceive the increased spectral information being provided by the implant as well as the variations in fo as coded by the stimulation rate delivered to a given channel, (tobey & hasenstab, 1991). these variations in fo were in turn providing increased suprasegmental information required in the use of questions, as was emphasizing certain words within a sentence in order to either alter the meaning of the sentence or emphasize a specific word within the sentence, (iler-kirk & edgerton, 1983). it is also interesting to note that the words in the sentences which were produced with the highest fo pitch remained consistent throughout all the auditory input situations. these words produced with the highest fo pitch were also the words produced with the highest amplitude peaks. these results indicate that the subject is utilizing the suprasegmental features of speech accurately with the aim of emphasizing a specific word in an utterance. segmental features vowels the overall decrease in vowel duration with the use of the cochlear implant is 'confirmed by the results obtained by tartter et al., (1989) who also found a decrease in vowel length in a postlingually deafened individual during the first year of usejwith a multichannel cochlear implant. the decrease in vowel length can be attributed to the improved temporal coding mechanisms provided by the cochlear implant, which in turn, improve and enhance auditory feedback monitoring, implicated in regulating the phonetic precision of segmental and suprasegmental characteristics of speech, (waldstein, 1990; svirsky et al., 1992). as far as the relationship between the vowel formant frequencies is concerned, the results of the f1 and f2 values across the various auditory input situations did not show any significant differences (table 5). the relationship between the f1 and f2 values for the eight different vowels indicated that the vowel,productions across the various auditory input situations occupy well defined loci as expected. these results verify the minimal changes in the relationship between f1 and f2 across the various auditory input situations, indicating relatively stable vowel productions. to further enhance the interpretation of these results, the mean f1 and f2 cochlear implant values were compared with the mean values obtained from normal hearing afrikaans speaking male subjects, (van der merwe et al., 1993). the mean f1 values obtained in the cochlear implant condition for each vowel were consistently higher than the normal f1 values. tartter et al., (1989) in their study indicated that their subjects exhibited lower values after a period of use with the cochlear implant. a possible explanation for the higher f1 values obtained in the present study, could be attributed to the fact that the subject was not a standard afrikaans speaker. variations from the norm are often found when a dialect of a language is spoken, (sommerstein, 1977). by taking the overall results into account, it can therefore be assumed that the subject's profound hearing loss did not result in deviant vowel productions. consonants with regard to the relative amplitude peaks for consonants in cvc word combinations across the various auditory input situations, the most significant increases in amplitude peaks were for the plosive sections of the stop consonants (table 6). these significant increases in amplitude peaks can be interpreted in terms of the absence, in the first instance, of the plosive releases in the no-amplification situations for the consonants / ρ /, / 1 / and / k / as well as the consonants / ρ / and / k / in the hearing aid situation, to the presence in the second instance of plosive releases for these consonants during the cochlear implant intervals. for the fricatives in the initial and final positions, the majority of amplitude peaks in either the 3or 6-month cochlear implant conditions are higher than the amplitude peaks observed in the no-amplification or hearing aid conditions, indicating improved accuracy in consonant production. this can be attributed to two factors. in the first instance, the cochlear implant is providing increased spectral information which in turn allows the subject to perceive these consonants auditorily, thereby improving the auditory feedback mechanism of the subject's own productions, (dorman, soli, dankowski, smith, mccandless & parkin, 1990). in the second place, the cochlear implant is resulting in increased accuracy in the articulation of consonants, which consequently results in an increase in the intensity of the production. the spectral frequency ranges of the plosive sections of the stop consonants during the 6-month cochlear implant interval did show a decrease in the frequency regions occupied for the majority of the productions when compared with the no-amplification condition. a decrease in frequency range is a direct result of a more concentrated release of the plosive section of the stop consonant, thereby improving the accuracy of productions. minifie (1973), mentions that the noise bursts of the stop consonants are dependent upon the volumes within the vocal tract, which participate in resonance of the source energies. these volumes are primarily determined by the vocal tract occlusion. it can therefore be assumed that the more concentrated production of the plosive sections of the stop sounds, can be attributed to the correct placing of vocal tract occlusion. the latter is a result of the improved auditory feedback mechanisms provided by the cochlear implant itself. as far as the fricatives are concerned, the spectral noise bands with the cochlear implant at the 6-month interval also showed a decrease in the frequency ranges measured for each fricative when compared to the nodie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 sandy cummings, emily groenewald, rene hugo, lida miiller, mike van der linde amplification situation. these spectral noise band limits measured for the fricatives fell within the noise band limits of normal speakers, (baken, 1987). the majority of the fricatives showed an increase in the minimum frequency with the use of the cochlear implant. these findings resulted in a narrowing of the spectral noise band frequencies which once again indicated the concentration of energy as a result of marked vocal tract constriction, resulting in increased accuracy of fricative production (minifie, 1973). listener analysis the results of the listener's rating of pitch variation indicated that the subject was beginning to use pitch and intensity information being provided by the cochlear implant. these results are supported by iler-kirk & edgerton, (1983); dowell et al., (1985); leder et al., (1986); and tyler & kelsay, (1990), who also reported improvements in voice control with the use of a multichannel cochlear implant. these improvements can be attributed to the pitch information being provided by the cochlear implant resulting in improved auditory feedback of the subject's utterances. as far as vocal duration and pausal duration are concerned, the improvements which were made could once again be attributed to the improvements in auditory feedback resulting in improved voice control and temporal resolution, (leder et al., 1986; tyler & kelsay, 1990; tartter et al., 1989). the latter improvements correlate well with the results obtained during the suprasegmental analysis. conclusion in conclusion, the results of the spectrographic analyses indicated that the information provided by the cochlear implant resulted in the improved use of suprasegmental features by the subject. there was an overall decrease in sentence length resulting in less drawn out speech. for the sentences where there was an increase in sentence length, the presence of consonants in the final position during the cochlear implant intervals as opposed to the absence thereof during the no-amplification and hearing aid situations, provided a logical explanation for this occurrence. the use of increased word stress measured in terms of relative amplitude as well as an increase in the variation of fundamental frequency, typically resulted in less monotone and more variable speech production. as far as the segmental features are concerned, the results of the vowel analysis indicated that the cochlear implant was providing improved auditory feedback which subsequently resulted in a decrease in vowel length over time. the relationship between the first and second formants for the various vowels did not show any significant differences across the various auditory input situations. these results were expected as the subject's vowel productions were not perceived as being deviant when subjectively compared to the same vowel productions by normal hearing individuals. the overall increase in relative amplitude peaks for the consonants investigated as well as the narrowing of the spectral frequency ranges and the spectral noise band frequencies indicated that the use of the cochlear implant was resulting in far more accurate consonant productions. the results of the listener's analysis of speech production indicated that the multichannel cochlear implant confirmed a significant improvement in the use of suprasegmental features in the subject's speech production as perceived by the listeners. the overall improvement in the use of suprasegmental speech features can be attributed to the normalization in the use of pitch variation as well as vocal and pausal duration with the cochlear implant over time. finally, the "hearing" provided by the cochlear implant is considered to have two major roles in maintaining the communicative effectiveness of the production mechanisms in adults. in the first instance, self hearing helps to calibrate production mechanisms by monitoring relations between the implant user's own articulations and his/her acoustic output. in the second instance, the speaker can validate his/her acoustic output by observing the behaviour of the listeners and by detecting discrepancies between his own speech and that of the listeners, (perkell et al., 1992). references baken, r.j. (1987). clinical measurement of speech and voice. massachusetts: collegehill press, inc. cochlear pty limited. (1989). mini system 22: audiologists handbook. sydney, australia. cohen, n.l., waltzman, s.b. & shapiro, w.h. (1985) clinical trials with a 2 2 c h a n n e l c o c h l e a r p r o s t h e s i s : in laryngoscope, 95: 1448-1454. coles, l.d. (1990). the effect of certain suprasegmental features of the hearing impaired child's speech intelligibility. unpublished master's thesis, university of pretoria. cowie, r., douglas-cowie, e. & kerr, a.g. (1982). a study of speech deterioration in post-lingually deafened adults. in journal of laryngology and otology, 96: 101-112. dorman, m.f., soli, s., dankowski, k., smith, l., mccandless, g. & parkin, j. (1990). acoustic cues for consonant identification by patients who use the ineraid cochlear implant. in journal of the acoustical society of america, 88: 2074-2079. dowell, r.c., brown, a.m., seligman, p.m. & clark, g.m. (1985). patient results for a multiple-channel cochlear prosthesis. in schindler, r.a. & merzenich, m.m. (eds.): cochlear implants. new york: raven press, ι dowell, r.c., seligman, p.m., blarney, p.j. & clark, g.m. (1987). speech perception using a two formant 22-electrode cochlear prosthesis in quiet and in noise|. in acta otolaryngology (stockholm), 104: 439-446. > economou, α., tartter, v.c., chute, p.m. & hellman, s.a. (1992). speech changes following reimplantation from a single-channel to a multichannel cochlear implant. in journal of the acoustical society of america, 92: 1310-1323. eddington, d.k. (1983). speech recognition in deaf subjects with multi-channel intracochlear electrodes. in parkins, c.w. & anderson, s.w. (eds.): cochlear prosthesis: an international symposium, annals of the new york academy of sciences, 405: 48-63. iler-kirk, k. & edgerton, b.j. (1983). the effects if cochlear implant use on voice parameters. in otolaryngologic clinics of north america, 16: 281-292. lane, h. & webster, j. (1991). speech deterioration in postlingually deafened adults. in journal of the acoustical society of america, 89: 859-866. leder, s.b., spitzer, j.b., milner, p., flevaris-phillips, c., richardson, f. & kirchner, j.c. (1986). reacquisition of contrastive stress in an adventitiously deaf speaker using a single-channel cochlear implant. in journal of the acoustical society of america, 79: 1967-1974. ling d. (1976). speech and the hearing-impaired child: theory and practice, washington, d.c.: the alexander the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c o e e c h p r o d u c t i o n c h a n g e s with the u s e of a m u l t i c h a n n e l cochlear i m p l a n t in a p o s t l i n g u a l l y h e a r i n g i m p a i r e d a d u l t 13 graham bell association for the deaf. minifie. f.d. (1973). speech acoustics. in minifie, f.d., hixon, j j. & williams, f. (eds.): normal aspects of speech, hearing and language. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, inc. n i c k e r s o n , r.b. (1975). characteristics of the speech of deaf p e r s o n s . in the volta review, 77: 342-362. perkell, j., lane, h., svirsky, m. & webster, j. (1992). speech of c o c h l e a r implant patients: a longitudinal study of vowel production. in journal of the acoustical society of america, 91: 2961-2978. schindler, r.a., kessler, d.k., rebscher, s.j., yanda, j.l. & jackler, r.k. (1986). the ucsf/storz multichannel cochlear implant: patient results. in laryngoscope, 96: 597603. s o m m e r s t e i n , a.h. (1977). modern phonology. edward arnold publishers. svirsky, m.a., lane, h., perkell, j.s. & wozniak, j. (1992) effects of short-term auditory deprivation on speech production in adult cochlear implant users. in journal of the acoustical society of america, 92: 1284-1300. tartter, v.c., chute, p.m. & hellman, s.a. (1989). the speech of a postlingually deafened teenager during the first year of use of a multichannel cochlear implant. in journal of the acoustical society of america, 86: 2113-2121. thielemeir, m.a., eisenberg, c.s. & brimacombe, j.a. (1982) audiological results with the cochlear implant. in annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology, 91, (suppl. 91): 6266. tobey, e.a. & hasenstab, m.s. (1991). effects of a nucleus multichannel cochlear implant upon speech production in children. in ear and hearing, 12: 48-54. tyler, r.s., gantz., b.j., mccabe, b.f., lowder, m.w., otto. s.r. & preece, j.p. (1985). audiological results with two single-channel cochlear implants. in annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology, 94: 133-139. tyler, r.s. & kelsay, d. (1990). advantages and disadvantages reported by some of the better cochlear implant patients. in the american journal of otology, 11: 282-288. van der merwe, α., groenewald, e., van aardt, d., tesner, h.e.c. & grimbeek, r,j. (1993). die formant-patrone van afrikaanse vokale soos geproduseer deur manlike sprekers. in south african journal of linguistics, 11(2): 71-79. waldstein, r.s. (1990). effects of postlingual deafness on speech production: implications for the role of auditory feedback. in journal of the acoustical society of america, 88: 2099-2114. a p p e n d i x a. r a t i n g s c a l e for the l i s t e n e r ' s e v a l u a t i o n of p i t c h v a r i a t i o n a n d v o c a l a n d p a u s a l d u r a t i o n description o f feature as perceived by listener 1 2 3 4 5 ω l-c pitch v a r i a t i o n m o n o t o n e little v a r i a t i o n normal too m u c h v a r i a t i o n a b n o r m a l v a r i a t i o n cd r n e ' talking to professionals the needler westdene organisation beg. no.ck87/02503/23 223 d.f. malan drive northcliff 2196 p.o. box 2739 northcliff 2195 ®(011) 888-1009/1087 fax: (011) 888-1186 telex: 4-2e068 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 36, 1989 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in language impaired and normal speaking four year old children marlene green b.a. (sp. & h. t h e r a p y ) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) summar y for the purpose of assessing a semantic feature theory of semantic acquisition in language impaired and normal pre-schoolers, semantic feature manipulation tests were administered to two language-impaired and two matched normal speaking four-year olds. results support clark's hypothesis that lexical acquisition proceeds from over-extended quasi-superordinate terms with few semantic features to more differentiated subordinate terms with a greater number of features. language impaired and normal children did not seem to be differentiated on these results. opsomming die volgorde van die aanleer van semantiese eienskappe is by taalgestremde en normale voorskoolse kinders nagegaan. 'n toets vir die manipulering van semantiese eienskappe is op twee taalgestremde en twee afgepaarde normale 4 jaar oue kinders toegepas. die resultate bevestig clark se hipotese dat leksikale aanleer geskied vanaf 'n baie bree algemene term met 'n paar semantiese eienskappe na 'n fyner gedifferensieerde onderverdeling van terme met 'n groter aantal kenmerke. daar is nie 'n verskil gevind tussen normale kinders en kinders met taalprobleme nie. up to the present time research in linguistic development has concentrated mainly on syntactic growth, β · 1 0 · 1 6 · 1 7 little emphasis has been placed on the acquisition of semantics (which is the system of meaning underlying language in the deep structure) although this system is gaining recognition as an important part of native-speaker competence. 3 · 4 · 1 4 · 1 7 while semantic acquistion plays an essential role in language growth, it also seems important in cognitive structure and growth. as bierwisch3 postulates, semantic universal might indeed form the basis for human perceptual and cognitive development. in semantic theory it is generally accepted that the units of meaning are lexical items. these are verbal concepts and comprise combinations of semantic markers which represent features abstracted from reality. 3 · 1 0 · 1 4 for example, "dog" is a lexical item comprising the features <+ animate > < human > < + 4 legs> <+ canine > etc. having defined lexical items as verbal concepts, it is necessary to examine the notion of concepts. from the literature the following four main generalizations can be d r a w n : 5 . 7 . 8 . 9 . i i . i 2 , 1 9 . 2 0 . 2 3 , 2 6 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaans verniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol22, deseber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 marlene green 1. concepts are the symbolic means whereby events which are discriminably different are rendered equivalent, due to their common incorporation of critical features, in order to form a class or category. the exemplars or members of the class formed in this way become differentiated from all other objects or events due to the presence (versus the absence) of the relevant critical features. therefore, "bananas", "meat" and "milk" which all contain the feature <+ edible> are classed together as exemplifying "food" in contrast to "paint" and "hat", which exclude this feature. 2. the extraction or isolation of features from a stimulus configuration, usually for the purposes of forming a concept, is termed abstraction. when abstracted features are used as the criteria of a category, generalization is said to occur. 3. the greater the number of criterial features, the more specific or concrete is the defined class. therefore, where there is a single criterial feature the defined class is broader and more abstract than any subordinate class (lexical item) with more features and hence greater definition. the class defined with a single criterial feature thus includes more exemplars. therefore, the concept "food", which has only the feature <+ edible>, is a broad superordinate concept. in contrast "fruit" also includes the features <+ tree-grown> < + natural> and lies subordinate to "food", although "fruit" in turn is broader than, and superordinate to "apple" which includes extra features ( <+ edible> <+ fruit> <+ apple tree> etc.) 4. concepts, because they focus on critical features, and ignore those which are irrelevant, are economic in terms of storage and retrieval in an efficient model of cognition. thus, in verbal concepts, the features which have been abstracted are termed semantic markers or features. it is the method whereby these are abstracted and generalized which concerns us in the study of semantic acquisition. until recently no satisfactory theory had been proposed on semantic acquisition. eve v. clark 1 0 , has however, attempted to integrate diarized observations and experimental evidence on word^meaning acquisition in young children into a conceptually feasible theory of semantic development. clark1 0 emphasizes that the number of extracted features determine the breadth of the defined category. she hypothesizes that verbal classes are first defined in early language acquisition by the presence of one semantic feature these pseudo superordinate classes are thus broad and poorly differentiated, and termed "overextended" by clark. 1 0 with the addition of differentiating , criterial semantic features during the development of the semantic system, the classes achieve greater definition and better symbolize reality. only when all differentiating features are incorporated into the meaning of the lexical entry, can further feature abstraction be achieved. at this later stage, when common features within a category can be abstracted, true superordinate concepts will develop. these true superordinates will have the fewest semantic features. thus the direction of semantic growth as hypothesized by clark,1 0 is vertical, from the overextended pseudo-superordinate (and often fictitious) class, downwards to the concrete, where the class is clearly defined by a great number of features abstracted from the environment. it is only when all the journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds 5 features are present in the lexicon of the concrete items that it is possible for the child to abstract what features are common to the items, and he is able to construct the valid vertical semantic hierarchy with correct and highly abstract superordinates, and progressively more concrete subordinates, stract superordinates, and progressively more concrete subordinates. in this way, clark 1 0 has clarified the central controversy of semantic acquisition theory, namely the direction of growth in semantic development. ! · 1 6 . 2 6 in addition her theory allows understanding of difference between adult and child word-meaning. these differences are thus seen as qualitative. further her theory lies in close agreement with vygotsky, 2 6 as both emphasize that the strategy used for abstracting semantic features from the environment closely affects the meaning of the word thus evolved. in accounting for this, vygotsky 2 6 postulates three phases in the acquisition of word-meaning: 1. syncretism: heaps of objects with arbitrary and unstable relationships are categorized together, and the words labelling these reflect the nature of the categorization. 2. complexes: concrete abstractions are made from the environment, often reflecting functional similarities. the words used reflect the concrete nature of the abstractions, though referring to the same referents as the adult words. this allows communication between adult and child. this strategy occurs in adults too, to some degree. 3. conceptual thought occurs when the child is no longer bound by concrete interrelationships. word meanings symbolize only the abstracted features as in adult thought. therefore, early in semantic development, words represent classes formed according to synthetic or complexive strategies. at this stage, the labels of focal items can be projected onto items associated with them perceptually. for example, "bow-wow", though originally representative of "dog" and "toy-dog", becomes attached to "buttons", "cufflinks" and small long objects. the features abstracted from the original, i.e. oblong shape or shiny surface, have caused generalization of the original label (vygotsky p. 70). later in development abstraction strategies approximate those used by adults, and the resultant word meanings approximate those of adult thought. thus, as the strategy affects word-meaning, it seems important to determine what strategies of abstraction the child used for attaching meaning to words. another dimension of cognitive mechanisms in semantic growth is emphasized by bloom4. she feels that language learning is semantically based. her con-itention is that syntactic relationships and the existence of the form-classes (e.g. noun, verb, adjective) result form the child's observations that certain words and their referents constantly appear in fixed relationships. the abstraction of these fixed relationships, allows words of the same form-class to be used interchangeably. language acquisition is achieved by the child correlating his experiential observations of relationship between agent, action and object, with the adult verbal input symbolizing these relationships. for example "the boy pats the dog" and "the girl eats the cake" symbolize the same tydskrif van die su-afrikaae veriin vir spraak en gehoorheelkde, vol. 22, decmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 marlene green relationship (agent, action, object), and adult verbal input confirms this relationship. if, as bloom4 suggests, language learning is semantically based, then, in the field of speech pathology, it is necessary to assess this aspect in children who have not learned language correctly. diagnosis for the individual child seems important, as areas of breakdown of linguistic learning seem to be individual· i z e d 2 · 2 1 . perhaps then for some language impaired children there is difficulty in abstracting relationships from the environment. the literature further suggests the presence of cognitive deficiency in children who learn language in a deviant manner. however, this deficiency has been inadequately defined 2 1 and requires further attention. from the preceding discussion it can be seen that there is a need for research into semantic theory and cognition for the following reasons: 1. a semantic feature acquisition hypothesis is as yet tentative and requires substantiation. 2. it is necessary to understand conceptual strategies of abstracting features from the environment as these seem to affect word-meaning acquisition. 3. as children with language impairment seem to have conceptual disorders, it is necessary to investigate whether this occurs in the area of abstracting meaning from the environment. method aims in view of the preceding rationale, the present study had as its aim the following: 1. to devise an adequate semantic test based on semantic feature abstraction and generalization in order to: (a) assess the reality of the feature acquisition theory of semantics and to (b) assess whether language impaired and normal children can be differentiated on this level. 2. to investigate cognitive strategies used in the abstraction of meaning in order to ascertain whether these differentiate language impaired and normal children. 3. to ascertain whether verbal concepts actively emphasized in the child's' environment are acquired in a manner different from those used less consciously or actively. further, it aimed to see whether differential experience with the words caused differences between the language impaired and normal children. subjects: two four year old boys with language impairment and two normal matched controls were used for the purpose of this study. in order to ensure maximum homegeneity within the sample for purposes of valid comparison, all subjects journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds 7 were selected according to the following criteria: 1. all subjects were four years old, as at this stage the language of children with normally developing language is contrasted markedly with that of children with language impairment. this differentiation is apparent because normal four-year-olds have correctly developed complex syntax and several transformations while language impaired children have developed few or deviant rules. diagnosis of language impairment is thus fairly certain at this age. while investigations into vocabulary development have utilized either very young children,10 or children older than six y e a r s , 1 , 1 6 none have attended to the age group between those — the four-year-olds. another factor motivating choice of four-year-olds is that rate of vocabulary acquisition is reported to peak at about four years, thereafter progressively decreasing with a g e . 1 7 2. male subjects were selected to control for certain differences in language acquisition found between males and females. males, as a group, seem to acquire language later and more slowly than females, during the first few y e a r s . 1 3 3. all subjects had older siblings. this was controlled for, as first-born children are reported to acquire language at a greater rate than their siblings.18 4. further all children were judged subjectively by the experimenter and the subjects' nursery school teachers to be of normal intelligence and to have no behavioural problems. these criteria were used in an attempt to control for factors other than linguistic ability which might influence performance on verbal, perceptual or conceptual tasks. 2 5 experimental subjects (li j a n d l i 2 ) : the essential criteria for selection of the experimental subjects were that they had been previously diagnosed as language impaired, and that they were presently attending speech therapy. experienced language pathologists at the university of the witwatersrand's speech and hearing clinic diagnosed the errors in the subjects' language as reflecting a deviation rather than a delay in linguistic acquisition. maturation would thus not remedy the errors, as would occur with language d e l a y . 1 8 control subjects (njand n 2 ) : each language impaired subject was matched with a male control subject from the same nursery school, who was closest in age to him. and who was regarded by the teacher to have normal language and intelligence. the linguistic ablity of all subjects was assessed in terms of its deviation or sophistication and an attempt was made to rank the linguistic ability of the subjects in relation to each other by the experimenter and an experienced language pathologist. lij was judged to have a greater degree of impairment than l i 2 , while njwas assessed as having less mature language than n 2 . table 1 summarises all variables where differences were found between the subjects. it is concerned with age, sibling position and linguistic ranking and description. tydskrif van die su-afrikaanse verenigin vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) marlene green su bj ec t ω oc < po si ti on in f am il y n um be r in f am il y r an ki ng o f l in gu is ti c a bi li ty description of linguistic output li j 4;3 3 3 4 multiple phonologic and syntactic deviations. vocabulary fair. n1 4 -2 2 2 2 normal syntax and phonology with developmental errors. vocabulary fair. li2 4;7 2 .3 3 syntactic and mild phonological deviations. vocabulary fair. n 2 4;7 4 4 1 sophisticated mature syntax and phonology. /s/ articulation error. vocabulary good. table 1: lntersubject differences tests used in the study a selection pretest was first administered (for purposes of subject selection) followed by the semantic feature manipulation tests. for purposes of clarification the tests administered will be discussed in terms of the rationale for their use, and a description of the test materials. pretest for selection purposes'a pretest was conducted to ensure that all subjects could categorize conceptually and to establish to which commands they responded best, in order to select the command most effective for subsequent testing. use was made of several three-dimensional objects exemplifying the conceptual categories of "dolls", "animals" and "fruit and vegetables". semantic feature manipulation tests two sets of material manipulating semantic markers, were constructed in / order to assess what features constitute the child's verbal concept, what ·' referent types he has present in his vocabulary, what contrasts he draws between referents, and how the semantic features selected relate to his experience. two superordinate concepts.were utilized for the purposes of this test — "furniture" and "containers". these superordinates were selected as the child has variable contact with these in his environment.through discussion with the parents of subjects, it seems that "furniture" is subject to active teaching by parents, whereas "containers" seem to be more passively taught. it was journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds 9 felt that there might consequently be a difference in the manner in which the child would form the concepts underlying these two superordinates. it was also felt that the less familiar class might yield interesting results which could possibly differentiate between language impaired and normal children in the way they learn passively defined concepts. in the adult lexicon both superordinates fall under the following hierarehic feature matrix. < + noun > < + entity > < + object > < + concrete > < + inanimate > < + common > we may not, however, assume that this is so in the child's lexicon. rather, word association studies with children have led to confusion about what features might be present. word-association studies cited by anglin1 show that young children tend to associate stimulus words with words of other form-classes, e.g. "ball" elicits the response "throw". however, the present writer feels that this heterogeneity probably results from the functional use of the words being grouped together in frequently used contexts. it is consequently felt that the conclusion drawn by anglin, 1namely that children do not have distinct form classes, is an invalid one. rather, in observing normal four-year-olds, we can assume that, as they make use of nouns interchangeably in similar semantic relations, nouns are functionally and syntactically equivalent for them. for example, because "cat" and "dog" are interchangeable in the same relations they can be assumed to belong to the same forrji-class (as drawn from bloom's 4 conclusions). thus it is felt that investigation into one form-class is appropriate for this age group, and removes confounding variables introduced byword-association tasks. the form-class of nouns has been selected in this study, as nouns' referents are easily represented in a manner which cannot be easily misinterpreted by the child — by models or pictures. models, then, are amenable to the manipulation of those physical features which are regarded as representative of possible semantic markers, and it is through the use of such models that it is proposed one can come to understand the concepts of referents which the child has labelled.15 tydskrif van die sukl-afrikaan.se verenigini! vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, descmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 marlene green as categorization into subordinate classes requires the abstraction of common criterial features, a categorization task can be used for investigating classmembership criteria — that is, what features constitute the concept..for the purposes of deducing the criterial semantic markers, a set of semantic markers was manipulated in terms of all possible interrelationships. the features common to all objects categorized similarly, are then the criterial features for that class. rosch 2 2 shows that each concept has focal and peripheral exemplars, due to its internal structure. focal items are those which most accurately incorporate the ideal features of the concept. these items are not ambiguous and can be interpreted only as exemplars of the original concept. peripheral items do not incorporate all the critical features of the concept and because of their insufficient definition can be incorporated into other concepts (being on the periphery between them). they are in this sense ambiguous. it was decided to determine these as it was felt that focal items would demonstrate those semantic features most typical of the class. the semantic features manipulated were perceptually present, three-dimensional physical features of the objects, e.g. back and seat. the use of visual stimuli and primarily nonverbal responses was motivated by findings that children demonstrated improved performance on such tasks, in contrast to those where stimuli and responses were verbal.1 , 5 description of semantic feature manipulation test materials: the features of eight types of furniture items present in the adult vocabulary were systematically manipulated in 127 wooden models. the furniture items selected for feature manipulation were: bed, table, desk, dressing-table, stool, chair, bench and couch. containers representing eight items focal in adult language were selected: cup, mug, glass, jug, jar, bowl, bottle and vase. only certain features which ran almost throughout all the types were manipulated e.g. lid. in all 47 real-size objects were used, because, in contrast to the furniture, models are difficult to construct, and items of real size are portable. procedure on all tests subjects were tested individually by the experimenter in quiet, familiar surroundings. the pretest was administered in their homes in order for experimenter to establish rapport with the subject and to eliminate unnecessary tension which affects test responses.7 the remaining tests were carried out in an isolated room at the children's nursery schools, except in the case of li j , who was tested at home due to unavoidable circumstances. in all test enviroments an attempt was made to decrease extraneous distracting stimuli to ensure maximal attention being directed to test materials. each testing session was broken by frequent short rest intervals (about three minutes) whenever subjects showed evidence of restlessness or fatigue, due to the limited concentration span of the four-year-old. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds 11 figure 1 systematic manipulation of semantic features shown on examples of the "bed" series fig. la. <+ 4 legs . > <+ top > <+ headboard > <+ bedding > <+ mattress > fig. lb. <+ 4 legs > <+ top > <+ headboard > c bedding > <+ mattress > fig. 1c. <+ 4 legs > <+ top > <+ headboard> <— bedding > <— mattress > fig. id. <+ 4.1egs > <+ top > < headboard > <+ bedding > <+ mattress > fig. le. <+ 4 legs . > <+ top > < headboard > < bedding > <+ mattress > _ fig. if. <+ 4 legs > <+ top > < headboard > <— bedding > <— mattress > tydskrif van die su-afrikaanse verenigin vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 marlene green feature manipulation tests before the feature manipulation tests could be administered, it was necessary to ensure that subjects could categorize in terms of the superordinates. four, two-dimensional photographs of each superordinate were required to be sorted into two groups (i.e. containers and furniture). the following tests were then performed: 1. free categorization: subjects spontaneously sorted firstly the furniture and then the containers, into classes. it was felt that such sorting would tap the superordinates relevant to the child. 2. forced categorization: subjects were required to categorize objects into classes labelled by the experimenter, to ascertain whether these concepts were present in the child's lexicon. 3. subjects were then required to select focal versus peripheral members of the forced categories. method of recording and analysing responses all responses were recorded by the experimenter in written form. the results were then analysed by examining the feature matrices of each item in the subject's grouping, and abstracting out the features common to the exemplars of the category. results and discussion in dealing with the results an attempt will be made to analyse and discuss the responses of the subjects qualitatively on each test. this will be followed by a general discussion of the important trends emerging from this study. i'retkst all subjects were able to supersede perceptual categorization and categorized according to named superordinates, although each subject used different methods of categorization. llj immediately attempted categorical sorting, grouping dolls together. he then sorted more primitively according to the common feature of colour/' (i.e. perceptually). although behaving inappropriately, he was unable to alter his mode of response until the experimenter prompted this by suggesting an alternative. he then sorted entirely conceptually, ignoring perceptual features. both subordinates and superordinates~were labelled, although the category "fruit and vegetables" was namely functionally — 'we eat them' ll2 demonstrated a categorical attitude, sorting according to superordinates. he seemed also to use complexive thought, as, after two yellow fruit items were grouped together, he began sorting perceptually, according to colour. when prompted, he again commenced conceptual categorization, including journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds 13 colour-categorization within classes. subordinates and superoidinates were labelled. n j sorted concretely (i.e. perceptually) according to colour at first, but when requested to change his mode of response, categorized conceptually. he was then able to name subordinates and labelled superordinates "dolls", "fruits" and "animals". n 2 first evidenced concrete sorting, grouping into sets of identical objects. however, responses immediately became conceptual when instructions were modified from sorting "things that are the same to things that are a little bit the same". superordinates were termed "dolls", "fruit we eat" and "animals." all subjects were selected for participation in feature manipulation tests on the basis of their ability to sort conceptually, especially with appropriate commands. feature manipulation tests as the results on these tests are too lengthy to present in full in a report of this nature, pertinent examples of typical responses will be presented in the frame-work of an analysis and discussion of the results. relationship between parent word-referent model and child's responses an examination of the data reveals that there is a close correlation between the child's knowledge of word-referent relationships and the amount of contact he has had with the word-referent relationship from his mother's speech. therefore, in those word-referent relationships very frequently encountered by the child, the feature specification was identical to that of the mother. less known relationships were seen to be less-adequately defined by the child (see figure 2). more actively taught relationships (i.e. "furniture") were apparently better known. free categorization in examining the results it is evident that free categorization generally led to the abstraction of objects into broad, ill-defined categories. these categories were labelled and their feature-specification incorporated few semantic markers. for example, the category "chair" (incorporating the features <+ back> <+ seat>) was seen to contrast with "table" (which incorporated <+ top> <+ legs>). in terms of the evidence that these categories were able to be subdivided into more specific categories, it seems that subjects spontaneously categorized into their superordinates. forced categorization, therefore, led to subjects sub-dividing the superordinates. the superordinate categories thus evidenced were more specific and incorporated a greater number of semantic markers including those general markers specifying the superordinate. for example, lij was able to sub-divide the superordinate "table" in the following manner — tydskrif van die su-afrikaanse verenigin vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 marlene green -tabletable ""<+ top > <+ legs > <— enclosed > <— mirror > <— drawers > desk <+ top > <+ legs > <+ enclosed > < i mirror > <— drawers > forced categorization the following generalizations may be drawn from the results: the child's method of categorization correlated strongly with his contact with word-referent relationships. thus the subjects showed greater ease in naming and categorizing conceptually with familliar word-referent relationships. this was seen in contrasting between "furniture and "containers" as well as within these general categories. from the forced categorization of objects, the following types of word-referent relationships emerged. ( see figure 2). figure 2. word-referent relationships found in the study known referent-label relationships superordinate (overextended) known subpartially known ordinates subordinates unknown referent label relationships categorized referents attached randomly to meaningless labels (li 2 , l i 2 ) ( n 1 ( n 2 ) categorized referents unattached to meaningless label (all (all subjects) subjects) the strategies of abstraction were closely affected by the type of word-ref-' erent relationship applicable. as can be seen from the above figure, all subjects had the ability to categorize conceptually with the meaningful stimuli. when they were familiar with the label applicable to the stimuli they sorted according to the correct superordinate. they were also able to show exemplars of familiar superordinates when asked to do so. however, with less familiar or less meaningful word-referent relationships (i.e. unknown referent-label relationships) the subjects were faced with the task of ascribing meaning to the unfamiliar objects, and this was achieved by the use of certain general strategies: journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds .15 the unfamiliar objecis were perceptually sorted or else sorted together into undifferentiated heaps. sometimes the language-impaired subjects labelled perceptually grouped items irt terms of an arbitrary unfamiliar label for example "stool" was randomly used to name a class of unfamiliar objects. normal children would not do so if the name was unfamiliar. where the label was familiar to the subjects but only partially meaningful in terms of its attachment to referents it was apparent that all the subjects were uncertain of the referents, as in the cases of "dressing table" and "desk" (see figure 3). in this event it seemed that an attempt was made by subjects to hypothesize sets of abstracted features for the labels, and all subjects became perceputally bound to the present stimuli. they-seemed to attempt to abstract out two sets of features, one set applicable to each label, each set of features being mutually exclusive. thus each subject reached a different feature specification, although all were seen to work from concrete comparisons see figure 3. n 2 , however, was the only subject to achieve totally exclusive feature specifications for these two classes even though his deductions led to categorizations different from those made by the adult. figure 3. feature specification deduced from subjects' differentiation of the partially known labels "dressing table" and "desk". li, li, table table "dressing "desk'" "dressing "desk" table" table" <+ top> <+top> <+ top> <+ top> <+ legs> <+ legs> <+ legs> <+ legs> <+ enclosed > <+ enclosed > <+ enclosed > <+ enclosed> <— mirror > < mirror> <— mirror> <— mirror> <+ drawers >_ <— drawers >_ <+ drawers>_ <— drawersx νχ table "dressing table" <+ top> <+ legs> <+ enclosed > <— mirror> <— drawers> "desk" <+ top> <+ legs> <+ enclosed > <— mirror> <— drawers > "dressing table" <+ top> <-+ legs> <+ enclosed > <+ mirror> <+ drawers> "desk" <+ top> <+ legs> <+ enclosed: <— mirror> <— drawers> tydskrif van die su-afrikaanse verenigin vir spraak en eoorheekunde, vol. 22 desmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 marlene green conversely, where a referent seemed appropriate to two sets of objects, subjects lij li 2 , and n j grouped these items perceptually. these items were, therefore, evaluated in terms of which item they closest resembled, and were named similarly. for example, in being presented with the object with four legs, and a padded top surface, these subjects compared this to certain items previously categorized as beds, because of their headboards. in a manner typical of complex thought, the feature of padding (or <+ mattress>) was abstracted, and the unlabelled item termed "bed". however, n 2 did not depend on perceptual stimulus-bound comparisons. rather he labelled this item a "table" drawing on his past experience, where the features of the present item conformed with the features of the schema for "table". while no conclusion about semantic acquisition may be drawn from the data on familiar subordinates, unfamiliar subordinates furnish interesting information. it is these subordinates which are in the process of developing and thus it is possible to discuss whether the process of development in the child with already established language, is in agreement with a semantic feature acquisition hypothesis. in order to assess this, it is necessary to examine the validity of the principle of overextension, as well as the direction of semantic growth on those developing word-referent relationships. for the purposes of this discussion, a developing word-referent relationship will be defined as a label whose semantic feature specification is incomplete in terms of the adult model, although approximating this e.g. with "desk" and "dressing table". from examination of the data two types of developing relationships are found: (a) where the adult model implies an incomplete feature specification (e.g. with li 2 the model word "cup" used by the mother implies incomplete specification as it refers to all drinking utensils see table iib); and (b) where the adult model incorporates all differentiating features (e.g. the parents of all subjects adequately contrast "desk" with "dressing table" and thus these model labels can be assumed to incorporate all semantic features). the several examples of such relationships seen in the data seem to demonstrate that all subjects follow a common trend. as an instance of type (a) it is seen that in all subjects "bench" and "couch" were not contrasted either in the adult model or the child's vocabulary item. the feature specification of these was uniform amongst all children, but the label attached conformed to the relevant adult's model. the feature specification was <+ back >] <+ seat > <+ 2 people> <+ legs > which excluded the feature differentiating between the two labels ("bench" and "couch") (i.e. <+ upholstery> or < upholstery>). thus both adult model and child word can be termed overextended, and not superordinate, journal of the south african speech a hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds . 17 ελ λ ό ελ λ 2 •g v •ΐί λ 5 ε +1 ν « ca ^ β.f j c ό "2 s " 8 | 5··° f £ ν ν 0} >ϊ •c ελ ελ 3 3 £ 1 c >. ϋ · » •s sp ο ζ υ rt c α ο ελ ο s c * § 2 = ^ s (ν •!a + λ •α £ s •ΐί ο * s 2 ·α c λ +1 ο &0 w υ e ν c ·3 λ σ · 2 2 5+1 £ t£3 v ™ >1 μ ε s ·δ+ι > λ v c α ) ελ (u ω >>~ι 4) ν ο >. .η -•8 >? 3-s s s §•§ g· s £ .s λ υ <υ rt ed ο •5 ο λ ο t-h c 3 σ λ λ ο ο 2 -s ο i o + " ν " v s a s a s2 ~ oj s2 ~ ω ~ ed 3 +1 +1 £ ν ν c ό 2 c 3 μ σ„ λ ο ό ^ 8 s 3 5 σ" . ο β λ ο .8 s > . £ ιττ v ο > ε λ ε ο i 2 v c ο λ ο ελ ^ e « t 1 " fs< r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) marlene green cvj 2 fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < + dr in k> < + o pa qu e> fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < +d ri nk > < + tr an sp ar en t > n o co nt ac t fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < + po ur > ' fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < +f lo w er s > fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < + po ur in g > v er y in fr eq ue nt c on ta ct in fr eq ue nt c on ta ct o nl y us ed in t er m s of " m ix in g bo w l" ih ζ fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < + dr in k> <+ op aq ue > n o co nt ac t n o co nt ac t fr eq ue nt w it h on ly o ne ex em pl ar < + po ur > v as e sy no ny m ou s w it h "b ow l" u se " bo w l" m or e fr eq ue nt ly fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < + po ur in g> n o co nt ac t in fr eq ue nt c on ta ct . in t er m s of " su ga r bo w l" o r "b ow l o f flo w er s" cvj 3 fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < +d ri nk > <+ op aq ue > fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < + dr in k > <+ tr an sp ar en t > n o co nt ac t fr eq ue nt < + po ur > fr eq ue nt c on ta ct fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < +p ou ri ng > n o co nt ac t in fr eq ue nt c on ta ct ih 3 fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < + op aq ue > < +d ri nk > fr eq ue nt c on ta ct < + dr in k > < +t ra ns pa re nt > in fr eq ue nt c on ta ct w it h co ffe e m ug s < sa uc er s> fa ir ly f re qu en t co nt ac t f < + p ou r> in fr eq ue nt c on ta ct fr eq ue nt ly u se d < +p ou ri ng > = ja r in fr eq ue nt c on ta ct w ith "t in " n o co nt ac t cu p g la ss m ug ju g v as e ο « ja r/ ti n/ c an b ow l journal ofthe south african speech and hearing association vol. 22 december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds 19 because the real superordinate would make use of a different label (e.g. perhaps "double chairs" as used by n 2 , or "furniture.") in terms of type (b) the explanations of the evolution of the terms "desk" and "dressing table" already proposed, seems also to support the semantic feature acquisition theory, as in all subjects an attempt was made to elaborate a detailed feature specification. in both types of evolving word-referent relationships the growth, seems, therefore, to proceed hierarchially in a downward direction — i.e. from generalized overextended terms to more differentiated, specific word-referent relationships, with nevertheless incomplete feature matrices. language-impaired and normal subjects were not able to be differentiated on these tests, except in terms of the unknown referent-label relationships as seen in figure 2. focal versus peripheral categorization known word referent-relationships: in all subjects it was seen that focal and peripheral exemplars included certain features which seemed to be essential to the exemplar being included in the class. these features were deduced by the experimenter to be the critical features of the class. these features, present in the focal exemplar but absent in the peripheral, were necessary for the object to be a realistic examplar of a class. however, even if these features were absent the item was categorized as a peripheral exemplar of the class if the critical features were present. for example all subjects contrasted focal and peripheral chairs in the following way: .chair focal chairs peripheral chairs <+ back > <+ seat > <+ 1 person > <+ legs >_ <+ back > <+ seat > <+ 1 person> <— legs > unknown word-referent relationships: all subjects were unable to differentiate focal and peripheral exemplars on any of these. general discussion the general discussion will be concerned with the implications and limitations of the present study. investigation is indicated into later semantic feature acquisition, in order to determine whether all subordinate terms (with completed feature specifications) must be acquired, before the child becomes able to abstract out supertydskrif van die su-afrikaanse verenigin vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 marlene green ordinate features and concept like <+ animate> and < animate> which are established later in semantic development.2 4 language-impaired subjects were seen to cope well when the word-referent relationships were familiar through frequent usage in their environments. therapeutically, this implies that vocabulary can be best learned when the lexical item is frequently paired with varying exemplars of the same category, and that attention should be drawn to the constant elements present in all the examples — i.e. the critical semantic markers. the limitations of this study lie mainly in its use of a small sample, which prevents the generalization of conclusions. furthermore, only a small aspect of semantic theory was investigated. such limitations imply the necessity for further research. the subjectivity of a qualitative analysis of responses, as well as the tediousness of detailed semantic marker manipulation testing, provide major criticisms of this study conclusion in conclusion, tasks of semantic feature manipulation reveal that there is reality for a semantic feature acquistion model, where the child draws firstly on predefined concepts from experience, and then from present perceptual data to attribute word-meanings to stimuli. words receiving conscious and active teaching are acquired with more definition than those learned less actively, though no differences seemed to exist between language-impaired and normal children on this level. references 1. anglin, j.m. (1970): the growth of word meaning. cambridge, massachusetts: m.i.t. press. 2. berry, m.f. (1969): language disorders of children: the bases and diagnoses. new york: appleton-century-crofts. 3. bierwisch, m. (1970): semantics. ch.l in j. lyons (ed.), new horizons in linguistics. penguin books. 4. bloom, l. (1973): one word at a time. the hague: mouton. 5. brown, r. (1956): language and categories. appendix in bruner, j.s., goodnow, j.j., and austin, g.a. a study of thinking. new york: john wiley and sons, inc. 6. brown, r. (1970): the child's grammar from i to iii. ch. 6 in r. brown. psycholinguistics: selected papers. new york: the free press. 7. bruner, j.s. goodnow, j.j., and austin, g.a. (1956): a study of thinking. new york: john wiley and sons inc. 8. bruner, j.s., olver, r.r., and greenfield, p.m. (1966): studies in cognitive growth. new york: john wiley and sons inc. 9. carrol, j.b. (1964): language and 77z0hgft?.englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall inc. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) semantic acquisition in four-yr. olds 21 10. clark, e.v. (1973): what's in a word? on the child's acquisition of semantics in his first language. in t.e. moore (ed.) cognitive development and the acquisition of language. new york and london: academic press, inc. 11. hoffman,.h.n. (1955): a study in an aspect of concept formation with subnormal, average and superior adolescents. genetic psychology monographs, 52,193-239. 12. johnson, d.m. (1972): a systematic introduction to the psychology of thinking. new york: harper and row. 13. johnson, w., darley, f.l., and spriestersbach, d.c. (1963): diagnostic methods in speech pathology. new york: harper and row. 14. katz, j.j. (1972): semantic theory. new york: harper and row. 15. lanham, l.w. (1974): personal communication. 16. mcneil, d. (1970): the acquisition of language: the study of developmental psycholinguistics. new york: harper and row. 17. menyuk, p. (1971): the acquisition and development of language. englewood cliffs, new jersey: prentice-hall, inc. 18. narun, l. (1973): speech pathology iii, lecture notes, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. 19. olson, d.r. (1970): language and thought: aspects of a cognitive view of semantics. psychological review, 77, (4) 257-273. 20. olson, d.r. (1970): cognitive development: the child's acquisitional diagonality. new york and london: academic press. 21. rees, n.s. (1973): auditory processing factors in language disorders: the view from procrustes' bed./. speech hearing disorders, 38(3), 304315. 22. rosch, e.h. (1973): on the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. in t.e. moore (ed.) cognitive development and the acquisition of language. new york and london! academic press, inc. 23. rosenstein, j. (1963): concept development and language instruction. exceptional children, 30, 337-343. 24. schaeffer, b., lewis a.e., and van decar, a. (1971): the growth of children's semantic memory: semantic elements. j. experimental child psychology, 11,296-309. 25. strauss, a.a.; and lentinen, l.e. (1950): psychopathology and education of the brain injured child. new york: grune and stratton. 26. vygotsky, l.s. (1962): thought and language. new york and london: m.i.t. press. tydskrif van die su-afrikaanse vereniginvir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, decmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) b e l l & h o w e l l l a n g u a g e m a s t e r s y s t e m teacher records the master track on a mams-powered language master. on a classroom .1727, pupil listens to the teacher's recorded voice. pupil makes his own recording, in pupil listens to his own recording, response to what the teacher has then compares it with the teacher's, said. if he is not satisfied, he can rerecord (re-recording automatically erases the previous pupil recording, but leaves the teacher's recording unimpaired). the inhnitely patient language master reading: in learning to read, listening is a key factor. by its unique combination of sight and sound, the language master system enables the child to look at a whole word or a phonic element within a word and at the same time hear the correct pronunciation on the teacher's track. speech t h e r a p y : because of the infinitely patient, individual attention it provides, the language master/' system is particularly useful in teaching both reading and speech to children or adults who are handicapped in some way (e.g. by aphasia, speech impediments, partial deafness). for f u r t h e r i n f o r m a t i o n c o n t a c t t h e language m a s t e r c o n s u l t a n t at g a l l o f o x in johannesburg. p.o. box 31884, braamfontein 2017. tel.: 28-6152. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acoustic reflex measurements a n d the loudness function in sensorineural h e a r i n g loss sheila uliel, m . a . ( a u d i o l o g y ) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) unit for hearing-impaired children, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary the suprathreshold acoustic reflex responses of forty two ears affected by sensorineural hearing loss of cochlear origin and fifty-eight ears demonstrating normal hearing, were recorded by means of an electro-acoustic impedance meter and attached x-y recorder. the recordings were done in ascending and descending fashion, at successively increasing and decreasing 5 db intensity levels from 90-120-90 db hl respectively, for the individual pure-tone frequencies of 500, 1 000, 2 000 and 4 000 hz. the contralateral mode of measurement was employed. analysis of these recordings indicated that the acoustic reflex responses could be differentiated into five characteristic patterns of growth, which could be depicted upon a continuum of peaked, peaked-rounded, rounded, rounded-flat, and flat shapes. the peaked and peaked-rounded patterns were found to predominate at all four pure-tone frequencies in the normal ears, while the rounded-fiat and flat patterns were found to predominate only at the higher pure-tone frequencies of 2 000 and 4 000 hz in the ears affected by sensorineural hearing loss. this latter relationship was also able to be applied to two disorders of the loudness function — loudness recruitment and hyperacusis. it was concluded that the flattened acoustic reflex patterns at the higher pure-tone frequencies constituted a potential diagnostic cue related to the differential diagnosis of sensori-neural hearing loss, and to disorders of the loudness function. opsomm1ng die bo-drumpel akoestiese refleks responsies van twee-en-veertig ore wat aangetas is deur sensori-nervale hoorverlies van cochlear oorsprong, en agt-en-vyftig ore met normale gehoor, is deur 'n elektro-akoestiese impedansmeter en aangehegte x-y opnemer vasgele. die opnemings is in stygende en dalende wyse, teen opeenvolgende toenemende en afnemende 5 db intensiteitvlakke van 90-120-90 db elk, vir die afsonderlike suiwerklank frekwensies van 500, 1 000, 2 000 en 4 000 hz gemaak. ontleding van hierdie opmetings bewys dat die akoestiese refleks responsies in vyf karakteristieke groeipatrone gedeel kan word. die groeipatrone kan weergegee word op 'n kontinuiim van puntig, puntig-gerond, gerond, gerond-plat en plat vorms. die puntig en puntig-geronde patrone is oorheersend gevind teen alle vier van die suiwerklank frekwensies in die normale ore, terwyl die gerond-plat en plat patrone slegs teen die hoer frekwensies van 2 000 en 4 000 hz in die ore aangetas deur sensori-nervale gehoorverlies oorheersend was. hierdie laasgenoemde verwantskap kan ook toegepas word op twee afwykings van die geluidskrag funksie — geluidheidsopbou en hiperakusie. die gevolgtrekking is afgelei dat die plat akoestiese reflekspatrone teen die hoer suiwerklank frekwensies as 'n potensiele leidraad kan dien, verwant aan die differensiele diagnose van sensori-nervale hoorverlies en afwykings van die geluidskrag funksie. /' / in recent years, there has been considerable discussion of a variety of suprathreshold auditory measures which have been used for both diagnostic and rehabilitative evaluations of patients with sensorineural hearing losses. j according to stephens et a l 1 9 a limited dynamic range of hearing, is in fact, the demonstration of 'loudness recruitment', which is traditionally die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) acoustic reflex threshold 59 used to indicate . . . a more-rapid-than-normal increase in subjective loudness for a given increase in physical intensity, (hirsch et al, ρ 213) 9 loudness recruitment has been conventionally accepted as being of great clinical value in pinpointing the locus of the sensorineural impairment. dix et al 8 were the first investigators to claim that the presence or absence of loudness recruitment would differentiate cochlear from retrocochlear pathologies, respectively. following on from their work, various procedures, both direct and indirect, were developed to determine the presence of loudness recruitment in the clinical situation. serious limitations have been noted with all the loudness recruitment procedures, with the exception of the metz recruitment test, developed in 1952. this test, which is considered to be both a direct and objective procedure, employs, as its basis, the fundamental principle of measuring changes in acoustic impedance at the eardrum, caused by the contraction of the acoustic (stapedius) reflex in response to sound. the acoustic reflex threshold is considered to be the most basic, static characteristic of the acoustic reflex (petersen and liden), and it describes the sensitivity of the fundamental acoustic reflex stimulusresponse function. it has been thoroughly investigated, and is currently well understood and extensively applied to clinical audiological diagnosis. for example, cochlear diagnosis utilises the narrowed relationship between the acoustic reflex threshold and the lowered audiometric threshold in the establishment of inner ear dysfunction and disordered loudness growth (or loudness recruitment) (metz). the dynamic properties of the acoustic reflex, which incorporate aspects of its response beyond threshold, have been the concern of investigators such as borg 4 and m 0 l l e r . 1 3 · 1 4 it is their contention that the suprathreshold growth of the acoustic reflex response amplitude in the auditory frequency — and temporal — domains and the interrelationship between this behaviour and different sound intensities, represents an important aspect of the dynamic behaviour of he acoustic reflex, which has not as yet been systematically applied to the clinical field. , _ retrocochlear diagnosis utilises the dynamic temporal response characteristics of the acoustic reflex, in that an abnormally rapid decay of the acoustic reflex to prolonged suprathreshold stimulation is indicative of auditory nerve dysfunction (anderson et al 1 · 2 ; sheehy and inzer ) cochlear diagnosis, however, does not utilise any of the knowledge gained from an understanding of the dynamic properties of the acoustic reflex, and relies exclusively upon examination of its static characteristics. _ it would appear that the amplitude regulation of the acoustic reflex, within the auditory frequency-domain, can offer much to the establishment of cochlear sensori-neural hearing loss diagnosis. ' ' ' ' i n e cochlear is involved with the analysis of the frequency and intensity characteristics of sound (resulting in the perception of pitch and die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 sheila uliel loudness), and disorders of the cochlear function interfere both with the hearing of certain frequencies (hearing loss), and with the loudness function (loudness recruitment). the acoustic reflex threshold has been shown to be related to the perception of l o u d n e s s , 3 ' 5 ' 6 ' 1 1 ' 1 7 but the relationship of its supra-threshold response characteristics to loudness perception requires detailed investigation. considerable variations in the growth of the acoustic reflex respugh all subjects had either a rising or flat audiometslope. details regarding the method of initial hearζ aid fitting were unavailable although all hearing is were fitted using general principles from the traional carhart approach. these subjects were fitted monaurally with behind-the-ear aids and although it is well established that a binaural fitting would have been more beneficial, many could only afford a single hearing aid due to financial constraints. the ages of the subjects ranged from 52 to 83 years with a mean of 72,3 years. none of the subjects showed evidence of central auditory processing deficits as suggested by a poor correlation between pure-tone findings and word recognition tasks, not by a significant deterioration in word recognition abilities in the presence of noise. pure tone and word recognition testing were conducted in an inter-acoustics company (iac) two-roomed sound-proof booth, using a grason stadler gsi-10 audiometer. sound-field frequency-modulated pure tones at 250 4000hz were generated and routed through the audiometer and amplified to a loudspeaker located in the sound booth. this speaker was also used for the word recognition testing. the audiometer, earphones, speakers and sound field had been calibrated to iso (1964) specifications four months previously. word recognition testing was conducted using the cid w22 phonetically balanced word lists developed by hirsch (1952). due to the unresolved controversy retable 1. calculation of gain and maximum power output a c c o r d i n g to pogo ( m c c a n d l e s s and lyregaard, 1983) the required insertion gain is calculated as follows: frequency (hz) insertion gain (db) 250 500 1000 2 0 0 0 3000 4000 0,5 htl 10 0,5 htl 5 0,5 htl 0,5 htl 0,5 htl 0,5 htl (where htl refers to hearing threshold level) the maximum power output (mpo) is calculated as follows : mpo (ucl 5 0 0 + u c l 1 0 0 0 + ucl2 0 0 0)/3 (where ucl refers to the uncomfortable loudness level) obtain frequency-modulated pure-tone thresholds for aided ear. calculate the require'd gain and output characteristics based on pogo. administer hearing aid questionnaire. determine aided sound-field thresholds. 4.1 calculate functional insertion gain. compare functional gain with required insertion gain. 5.1 functional gain within specified limits 5.2 functional gain not within specified limits check aided ucl 6 check aided ucl speech discrimination in quiet and noise. 7 speech discrimination testing in quiet and noise. 8 refer subject to hearing aid acoustician for modification if necessary. 8.1 subject returns to clinic after two weeks adjustment period. 8.2 return to step 3 above and proceed. figure 1. flow diagram of the administrated test procedure die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol: 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 sandra thorpe & carol jardine garding 25 or 50 word testing, the entire list of 50 words was administered (schmitz, 1980). a hearing aid questionnaire developed by maclean (1988) was administered to assess the effectiveness of hearing aid performance in the reduction of the hearing handicap (refer appendix a). the subjects were not permitted to peruse the questions before answering, in order to avoid premeditated responses. test procedures figure 1 provides a diagrammatic representation of the procedure followed. 1. pure tone audiometry a pure tone audiogram was established for each subject's aided ear at 250, 500, 1000, 2000, 3000 and 4000hz. 2. calculate gain and maximum power output requirements the required gain and maximum power output (mpo) according to pogo was determined as indicated in the table below (mccandless & lyregaard, 1983). 3. hearing aid questionnaire subjects evaluated their aids subjectively by means of a hearing aid assessment questionnaire (maclean, 1988) (appendix 1). due to statistical reasons related to the small population size it was not feasible to devise a rating scale. 4. aided sound-field audiogram an aided sound-field audiogram was obtained using frequency-modulated pure tones as recommended by mccandless & lyregaard (1983). the subject was seated at 0 degrees azimuth at a distance of one metre from the speaker. the non test ear was muffed with an earphone to prevent its influence on test results. 4.1 calculate required functional gain functional gain was determined by calculating the difference between unaided and aided thresholds at each frequency. 5. comparison of actual functional gain with required gain the criteria for acceptance of the hearing aid fittings were adapted from mccandless & lyregaard (1983). deviations between the measurements in the region 500 2000hz should not exceed 5dbs to constitute an acceptable hearing aid fitting. occasionally a lodb deviation is felt to be unavoidable and larger deviations are acceptable if they occur at all frequencies and in the same direction as this may be adjusted by the volume control. 5.1 functional gain within specified limits if the subject's functional gain met those specified by pogo, the hearing aid fitting was considered appropriate. 5.2 functional gain not within specified limits if functional gain was not within specified limits, the fitting was not considered appropriate and the subject was referred for further modification and testing. 6. checking uncomfortable loudness levels (ucl) to determine whether the maximum power output (mpo) of the hearing aid is appropriately set, mccandless & lyregaard (1983) recommend that the procedure incorporate testing to ensure that the ucl is not exceeded. this involved turning the hearing aid control full on and gradually increasing the level of narrow band noise at 1000hz. the mpo adjustment is considered satisfactory if the level of narrow band noise can be turned up beyond 80dbhl without reaching the subject's ucl. 7. word recognition testing "the purpose of a hearing aid is to enable hard of hearing subject to hear sounds that he cannot otherwise hear but desires to hear particularly the human voice ... (it) must make speech intelligible ..." (davis et al., 1946 as cited in pascoe, 1985). as difficulty in understanding speech is often the primary complaint in hearing impaired persons (martin, 1975 as cited in downs, 1982), a comparison of unaided and aided performance is an integral part of the hearing aid evaluation procedure. although not included in the prescriptive approach, the importance of word recognition testing cannot be easily dismissed because of its intuitively high face validity. unaided and aided word recognition testing was administered in a sound field situation. the subject was seated at 0 degrees azimuth at a distance of one metre from the speaker . both the primary message (speech) and the c o m p e t i n g message (speechweighted noise) were routed through the same speaker. monitored live voice was selected as the presentation mode to allow for the variations in response time required by the individual subject and to facilitate reinstruction or reinforcement where necessary (hodgson, 1987). word recognition testing was administered in both quiet and noisy situations. the use of background noise represents an approach that allows some realistic prediction about a person's functioning in real life noisy c o m m u n i c a t i v e settings (schmitz, 1980). a signal-to-noise ratio of odbhl, utilising speech-weighted noise was selected asjrepresentative of the most difficult listening condition encountered (lawrence et.al., 1976 cited in bress and bratt, 1977). , a level of 65dbhl was selected to conduct word recognition testing. whilst this level overcame! the stressful effects of having speech presented at a level too soft for the subject to respond to, as reported by most of the subjects, it nevertheless served as an appropriate level at which the subjects experienced substantial difficulty unaided (hodgson, 1986). 8. referral to the hearing aid acoustician the stipulated conditions for modification were that only earmould, tone control, gain settings and mpo changes be implemented to adjust the response of.the aid and that a new hearing aid would not be recommended. ^ subjects were required to wear their hearing aids for at least two weeks following modifications. this time period allowed the subjects the opportunity to use the modified aid extensively within their everyday routine, thereby enabling them to comment on / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the efficacy of the prescription of gain/output (pogo) in fitting hearing aids to mild and moderate sensorineural hearing losses 69 the new set of electroacoustic characteristics. subjects were then required to re-evaluate their aids subjectively using the hearing aid questionnaire. finally, a new sound field audiogram was obtained to determine whether gain requirements had been met. word recognition testing was then readministered. results and discussion all results should be considered in the light of the small sample size which had a direct bearing on the statistical procedures which could be applied. although trends have been established, all results should be seen as largely qualitative and not quantitative. in those circumstances where statistics cannot be applied at all, a descriptive analysis will be provided. 1. comparison between actual and required functional gain as shown in table 2, the extent to which the ideal and actual functional gain differ ranges from 0,4dbs at 500hz to 17,ldbs at 4000hz. according to mccandless & lyregaard's (1983) criteria for acceptance, only the gain at 250 and 1000hz represents acceptable fittings i.e., deviations between the actual and ideal do not exceed 5dbs at these frequencies. all these patients were originally fitted for amplification using the general principles of the modified comparative approach suggested by carhart (1946), although exact details were unavailable. there is much controversy about the importance of the high frequencies in speech intelligibility abilities. mccandless & lyregaard (1983) state that the hearing aids' frequency response should predominantly fit the table 2. the aided mean of ideal and actual functional gain at each frequency, and differences between them before modifications. hz 250 500 1000 2000 3000 4000 required gain (db) 5,4 13,3 21,7 25,4 27,1 25,4 actual gain (db) 5,0 6,7 17,5 14,2 15,0 8,3 delta fg (db) 0,4 6,6 ' 14,2 11,2 12,1 17,1 table 3. the aided mean of ideal and actual functional gain at each frequency, and the difference between them after modification. hz 250 500 1000 2000 3000 4000 required gain (db) 4,5 13,0 21 26 i 27 25 actual gain (db) 9,0 14,0 22 21 22 17 delta fg (db) 4,5 1 1 5 5 8 region of 250 to 2000hz. for frequencies above 2000hz, the requirements should be met as far as possible but are not as important as the lower and middle frequencies. a large discrepancy between ideal and actual gain means at 3000 and 4000hz is evident. however, pascoe (1985) comments that the critical range of frequencies which have a significant effect on word recognition scores, particulary those in noise, are those between 2500 and 6300hz. the relevance of this statement will be discussed later, however, it is important to note that this region is critical for speech intelligibility. as the difference between the ideal and actual functional gain is the basis for which an aid is accepted as a good or poor fitting, none of these aids would be accepted as appropriate and thus modifications were recommended for all subjects. subject dissatisfaction is often related to unsatisfactory mpo settings, therefore these were evaluated. according to mccandless & lyregaard's (1983) mpo specifications, all subjects' mpo levels were appropriate and thus did not require any adjustments. only five of the six subjects were prepared to have their hearing aid response altered by means of modifications to their earmoulds and/or tone control/gain settings. the results following modification of the hearing aids of these five subjects are presented in table 3 below. all subjects were using standard earmoulds and bores. although minor variations in earmoulds will not significantly alter the acoustical properties in the region 250 to 3000hz (mccandless & lyregaard, 1983), improvement of high frequency amplification from an increase in the inner diameter of the sound canal from the hook of the hearing aid to the tip of the earmould has been reported in the literature (ringdahl, leijon, liden & backelin, 1984). the use of a horn also provides an increase in functional gain at 3000 and 4000hz (hodgson, 1986). as these are the frequencies where the most modification was necessary, their implementation was discussed with the hearing aid acoustician. table 4. unaided word recognition scores in quiet and noise conditions subjects a β c d ε f quiet 20% 6% 16% 26% 32% 30% noise 24% 6% 10% 24% 10% 24% table 5. aided speech discrimination scores in quiet and noise before and after modification to the hearing aid. subject a β c d ε f before quiet 56% 46% 54% 54% 46% 36% noise 36% 18% 34% 42% 34% 24% after quiet 68% 64% 66% 80% 48% noise 42% 30% 52% 40% 34% die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 sandra thorpe & carol jardine however, strategies to improve the hearing aid response were left to the acoustician's discretion. as it was stipulated that a new hearing aid could not be recommended, it is important to bear in mind the limitations on the extent to which the hearing aid's response can be modified. table 3 indicates that a great improvement in functional gain was obtained across all frequencies. accordingly, all fittings except at 4000hz would be considered acceptable by mccandless & lyregaards' (1983) standards. shapiro (1976) stated that the predicted gain at 4000hz is the most difficult to attain. the significance of the negative numbers in the table is that too much gain was provided at those frequencies, i.e., over-amplification occurred, but these values are still within the acceptable range. 2. word recognition testing results of the word recognition testing are presented below (table 4) : the relevance of unaided word recognition scores and the benefits from aiding are particularly significant. hodgson (1986) states that poor discrimination ability is indicative of poor hearing aid candidacy and substantiates this statement by adding that individuals with unaided discrimination scores below 50% cannot expect to follow c o n v e r s a t i o n even with amplification (williamson & webber, 1985). hence, none of these subjects are candidates for successful amplification. a comparison of word recognition scores in quiet and noise, before and after modifications is illustrated in table 5. the criteria for a good hearing aid fitting in terms of word recognition scores is 80% or above according to ewerston (1966) and niemeyer (1969), thus only subject e's response in quiet conditions can be considered a good fitting. a scatterplot of word recognition scores in both quiet and noisy situations versus delta functional gain (delta fg), where delta fg refers to the modulus of the difference between actual and ideal fg generated and a definite trend noted. linear regression lines are as indicated in figure 2 below. (δ) delta insertion gain (db) • quiet • noise quiet noisy figure 2. linear regression of word recognition scores versus delta functional gain. (a) quiet conditions there appears to be a general trend towards improved word recognition scores as delta fg decreases. this suggests that pogo could provide some acceptable criteria for hearing aid use in quiet situations. a correlation coefficient of 0,79 confirms an acceptable correlation between word recognition scores and delta fg and is statistically significant. the regression line for the quiet condition is more reliable than that generated in noise. an r-square value of 63,39 implies that the regression curves account for 63% of the variation in the data. the correlation coefficient of 0,79 confirms an acceptable correlation between word recognition scores and delta fg. the f-ratio of 15,6 also describes the significance of the curve as a whole. (b) noisy conditions there is a far weaker trend towards improvement of word recognition in noise, and decreases in delta fg. indeed with regard to the regression line obtained for noisy conditions, only 29% of the variation is explained by the regression line i.e., r-square value of 29,49. this slope cannot be regarded as statistically significant. the curve could, however, still intuitively be considered significant and shows the kind of trend expected. however, these results are not convincing and may be due to the fact that required gain was not always successfully met at 4000hz as illustrated in table 3. alternatively, pogo may not be providing sufficient high frequency amplification as considered essential by pascoe (1985), or does not limit low frequency amplification to prevent the upward spread of low frequency masking in the presence of background noise. in summary, the regression line for quiet conditions supports the argument that as the delta fg decreases so word recognition scores improve. in addition, it acknowledges that a relationship does exist. on the other hand, the regression line for noisy conditions does not support this argument convincingly although a slight trend can be seen. intuitively some relationship does seem to exist since statistically the slope of this line cannot be construed as being zero. the correlation coefficient and r-square values support the latter statement. | it is evident from the above analysis that a closer approximation to ideal functional gain measurements as prescribed by pogo results in an improvement in word recognition scores, especially in quiet conditions. a disappointing correlation was found in noisy situations but it is anticipated that a closer approximation of ideal gain at 4000hz could improve results. welzl-muller & sattler (1984) note that patients whose hearing defects already cause a marked deterioration of word recognition in noise without a hearing aid, are considerably impaired even with a hearing aid. this may account for the poor relationship between word recognition scores in noise and delta fg. moreover, the central processing deficits in the geriatric ^population have been well documented and the difficulties encountered in noise is particularly noteworthy (staab, 1993). the results should, however, be viewed critically because of the poor test-retest reliability of word recognition testing and the high probability that differences in performance, could be due to chance and not changes in electroacoustic characteristics of the hearing aid the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the efficacy of the prescription of gain/output (pogo) in fitting hearing aids to mild and moderate sensorineural hearing losses 71 (schwartz & walden, 1983). hence, it is difficult to distinguish whether changes in word recognition scores are due to a closer approximation of functional gain values with pogo or the unreliability of word recognition measures. 3. subject satisfaction subject satisfaction was determined from responses to question 1 of the hearing aid questionnaire (refer appendix 1), taking into account questions 7 to 10. the former gave an indication of the subject's fundamental feeling about the hearing aid, whilst the latter determined specific circumstances of communication difficulty, benefits derived from the hearing aid and the necessity for further changes to the aid. satisfaction was ranked as 0 for happy and 1 for unhappy. in order to determine the relationship between satisfaction and delta fg, the following procedure was administered. delta fg was ranked as 0 for small differences and 1 for large differences. a grand mean of means of the differences in fg and satisfaction was taken for 11 data sets i.e., six subjects before and five subjects after modification. delta fg was compared to this grand mean. two of these differences were excluded from the analysis as their closeness to the grand mean implies that they were indeterminate differences i.e., neither small nor large. with the remaining nine data sets, a correlation matrix was drawn up showing an apparent correlation between satisfaction and delta fg. a ranking of 0 represents small delta fgs or good satisfaction with the hearing aid whilst a ranking of 1 shows that large differences exist between required and functional gain or that the patient was dissatisfied with the hearing aids. table 6 reflects that in 4 cases where the difference between actual and required gain is small, the subject is happy with his hearing aid. conversely, 3 cases show that large differences are associated with dissatisfaction. a spearman's correlation coefficient of 0,55 was recorded for correlation within this matrix. these results show a· general correlation between improved user satisfaction and smaller delta fgs but j [satisfaction 0 1 row total delta (δ) insertion gain 0 4 80 1 25 5 55.8 1 1 20 3 75 4 44.4 column 5 3 9 total 55.6 44.4 100 table 6. a correlation matrix depicting the relationship between satisfaction and delta functional gain they are disappointing. hearing aid fitting using the pogo solely does not appear to be sufficient to ensure user satisfaction and supplementary methods of hearing aid evaluation are therefore indicated. according to mccandless & lyregaard (1983) in the final analysis it is the patient who has the last word in hearing aid fitting as it is he/she who must live with the hearing aid in daily life. thus, from the above analysis we can tentatively suggest that a close approximation of hearing aid reponses to pogo results in a more satisfactory fitting. conclusions although the sample size was too small to allow any meaningful interpretation, there appears to be some correlation between the magnitude of difference in delta fg, scores obtained on word recognition tests and the user's satisfaction with the hearing device. it appears that as differences between functional and required fg decreases, so speech intelligibility and satisfaction improves. in the light of the afore-mentioned statements, we can tentatively suggest that the application of pogo appears to result in more acceptable hearing aid fittings and that it is fulfilling a very necessary requirement. it seems to be best suited to providing the basis for further modifications, i.e., it is a starting point at which to prescribe frequency and gain characteristics of the hearing aid as one can intuitively assess that improvements in gain will result in better speech intelligibility and greater satisfaction. the weak trends, however, suggest that pogo cannot be used solely but should be used with supplementary methods of hearing aid evaluation. this is particularly true if the hearing aid is to be used in noisy situations for large periods of time. in conclusion, "there is no single standard of hearing aid selection" (pascoe, 1986). however, the prescription of gain/output appears to be a valid predictor of the electroacoustic characteristics of a hearing aid, necessary for improved speech intelligibility and satisfaction. there are therefore direct implications for the introduction of this hearing aid fitting procedure to replace the modified carhart comparative approach as a gross hearing aid selection procedure, at least in patients presenting with mild and moderate sensorineural hearing impairments. these results also suggest that pogo may be used on other h e a r i n g i m p a i r e d populations other than the one researched. acknowledgements the authors wish to express their sincere thanks to mr d. smith of acoustimed hearing services for his time, willingness and assistance in earmould and instrument modifications. references angelo, r. & miller, g. 1988. selecting hearing aid gain and frequency response characteristics based on a comparison of loudness judgments and pogo, seminars in hearing, 9, 3, p.183-195. berger, k.w. 1991. introduction to three current hearing aid fitting methods, the american journal of otology, 12, 1, 40-44. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 s a n d r a t h o r p e & c a r o l j a r d i n e berger, k.w., hagberg, e.n. & rane, r.l. (1984). prescription of hearing aids : rationale, procedures and results, 4th edition, ohio : herald publishing house. bress, f. & bratt, g. (1977). special alterations of hearing aids and their effects on speech intelligibility. in larson, v., egolt, d., kirlin, r. & stile, s., auditory and hearing prosthetics research, new york : grune and stratton. buerkli-halevy, o. (1988). a guide to practical application of preselection formulae, phonak focus, 6, p.1-17. carhart, r. 1946. tests for the selection of hearing aids. laryngoscope. 56, 510-526. chasin, m.n. 1988. a clinically adaptive hearing aid fitting procedure, seminars in hearing, 9, 3, p.207-215. downs, d. (1982). effects of hearing aid use on speech discrimination and listening effort. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 47, 189-193. ewerston, h. 1966. the fitting of hearing aids in the danish hearing rehabilitation centres. audiology, 5, 2, p.384-391. hawkins, d., montgomery, α., prosek, r. & walden, b. 1987. examination of two issues concerning functional gain measurements. journal of speech and hearing disorders 52, 58-63. hirsch, i.j. 1952. the measurement of hearing. new york : mcgraw-hill book company. hodgson w. 1986. hearing aid evaluation : chapter 7. in hodgson w., hearing aid assessment and use in audiologic habilitation, 3rd ed., baltimore : williams and wilkins co. hodgson, w. 1987. basic audiologic evaluation. malabar : robert ε krigler publishing company. maclean, m. 1988. application of the national acoustics laboratories' hearing aid selection procedure and a new hearing aid assessment questionnaire. seminars in hearing, 9, 3, 173-181. mccandless, g. & lyregaard, p. 1983. prescription of gain/ output (pogo) for hearing aids. hearing instruments, 34, 1, 16-21. niemeyer, w. 1969. difficulties and effectivity of hearing aid equipment in presbyacusis. audiology. 8, 4, 535-538. northern, j. 1993. hearing aids, university of stellenbosch and south african speech-language-hearing association conference, somerset west, october. pascoe, d. 1985. hearing aid evaluation : chapter 46. in katz, j. (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology, 3rd edition, baltimore : williams & wilkins. ringdahl, α., leijon, α., liden, g., & backelin, l. 1984. hearing aid prescription rules using insertion gain measurements. scandinavian audiology, 13, 211-218. rose, d. 1977. hearing aid evaluation historical and present day perspectives. in larson, v., egolt, d, kirlin, r. & stile, s., auditory and prosthetic research, new york : grune and stratton. schmitz, h.d. 1980. hearing aid selection for adults : chapter 5. in pollack m.c. (ed.). amplification for the hearingimpaired, 2nd edition, new york : grune and stratton. schwartz, d. & walden, b. 1983. speech audiometry and hearing aid assessment: a reappraisal of an old philosophy. in konkle, j. and rintelmann, j.,perspectives in audiology series : principles of speech audiometry. baltimore : university park press. shapiro, i. 1976. hearing aid fitting by prescription. audiology, 15, 163-173. staab, w. 1993. hearing aid fittings in the geriatric population, society of hearing aid acousticians symposium, johannesburg, august. welzl-muller, k. & sattler, k. 1984. signal-to-noise threshold with and without hearing aid. scandinavian audiology, 13, 283-286. williamson, d.g. & webber p.j. 1985. hearing aid selection for adults : a review, audiological acoustics, 24, 1-16. a p p e n d i x 1 : h e a r i n g a i d a s s e s s m e n t q u e s t i o n n a i r e (maclean, 1988, p. 179) patient's n a m e : y e s n 0 1. a r e y o u h a p p y w i t h your h e a r i n g aid? 2. is it p h y s i c a l l y comfortable? 3. do y o u h a v e difficulties : a. i n s t a l l i n g it? b. o p e r a t i n g it? c. c l e a n i n g it? d. w i t h feedback (squealing or w h i s t l i n g ) ? 4. do other people's voices s o u n d p l e a s a n t ? 5. is the s o u n d of your o w n voice o k a y ? 6. do y o u h a v e difficulty w i t h the p h o n e ? 7. before y o u r e c e i v e d the aid w h a t w a s your m o s t t r o u b l e s o m e listening situation? 8. in that s i t u a t i o n , has the aid h e l p e d ? 9. has the aid h e l p e d in other s i t u a t i o n s ? 10. w h i c h l i s t e n i n g situations still pose p r o b l e m s ? 11. a r e you satisfied w i t h b a t t e r y life? 12. how m a n y h o u r s per day do y o u w e a r the aid? 13. in general are y o u getting g o o d v a l u e for the m o n e y spent on the aid? 14. would y o u r e c o m m e n d that a h e a r i n g h a n d i c a p p e d friend or relative try one? 15. a d d i t i o n a l c o m m e n t s . / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 51 methodological considerations in employing the continuous discourse tracking procedure with hearing-impaired adults l. le roux, ba (pretoria) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg r.w. turton, ma (witwatersrand) department of psychology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract this article addresses the various methodological issues involved in the use of the continuous discourse tracking (cdt) procedure as a test instrument for evaluating communication efficiency in hearing-impaired adults. an overview of the potentially confounding variables associated with the cdt technique is provided and consideration isgiven to the reliability of this procedure with reference to areas requiring systematic investigation. it is proposed that the adoption of an alternative paradigm as well as interdisciplinary approach to test construction may facilitate a multi-dimensional perspective to the assessment of speechreading and communicative ability in the hearing-impaired population. opsomming hierdie artikel bespreek die verskillende metodologiese aspekte inherent aan die gebruik van die "continuous discourse tracking" prosedure as 'n meetinstrument by die evaluering van kommunikasie-effektiwiteit by gehoorgestremde volwassenes. 'n teoretiese oorsig van die moontlike kontaminerende veranderlikes wat met hierdie tegniek gepaardgaan, word verskaf. vervolgens word die betroubaarheid van hierdie instrument indringend beskou met verwysing na navorsingsareas wat sistematiese ondersoek verg. daar word voorgestel dat die aanvaarding van 'n alternatiewe paradigma sowel as interdissiplinere pogings wat betref toetskonstruksie, 'n multidimensionele benadering tot die evaluasie van spraaklees en kommunikasievermoens by gehoorgestremdes kan bevorder. introduction recent advanced technology has introduced a new era in intervention strategies with profoundly hearing-impaired individuals, particularly for those postlingually deafened. south africa has recently inaugurated a multi-disciplinary approach to the assessment and rehabilitation of these individuals (miiller, 1988) which has sparked joff general interest in the potential benefits of cochlear implants in particular. for the audiologist, whose role is crucial, these developments present exciting challenges. t h e profession is increasingly called upon not only to assess candidates for cochlear prostheses, but also to recommend suitable alternatives, document the effectiveness of sensory devices and to provide for essential after-care services. these considerations, coupled with the current focus on quality assurance in clinical practice, emphasize the importance of accurate measurement of speech communication in the profoundly hearing-impaired. in addition, there is an urgent need to adapt currently available test instruments and to construct test stimuli to meet local demands for culturally appropriate diagnostic tools. the objective of this paper is to consider critically one of the speechreading assessment techniques, namely the continuous discourse tracking procedure, which has been widely applied to the assessment and treatment of postlingually, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykiniis, vol. 37. 1990 profoundly hearing-impaired persons. an appreciation of this technique is essential in order to avoid mechanical application of this procedure and to encourage clinicians to become discerning test users. the assessment of speechreading ability in reviewing the literature on speechreading assessment it is notable that, despite the absence of a conclusive theoretical framework for speech information processing, many innovative and radically different approaches have appeared during the last decade (e.g. boothroyd, 1987 (cited in montgomery & demorest, 1988); cronin, 1979; middelweerd & plomp, 1987). the absence of a viable model of speech perception has, nevertheless, exerted a noticeable influence on the standard of speechreading instruments developed so far; montgomery & demorest (1988) view most of these as being of inferior quality and describe them as not meeting current psychometric requirements. this statement needs some qualification however; it pertains specifically to the insufficient availability of psychometric data on existing measures, rather than to the inadequacy of the instruments per se. fortunately, as a result of several developments, most researchers are positive about improvements in the standard of speechreading instruments. they base their optimism on the following: θ sash a 1990 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a legacy of sound advice and equipment philips hearing aids audiometers fm systems head office: 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street p.o. box 3069, johannesburg 2000. tel: (011) 337-7537. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) methodological considerations in continuous discourse tracking procedure w i t h hearing-impaired adults 53 1) the recent strides which have been made in terms of model development (massaro, 1987); 2) the advent of interactive videodisc technology which has opened up avenues for exploring new methods of testing and 3) the availability of test construction and evaluation techniques which hopefully will be increasingly used by experimenters in this field during the stages of test development. at present, however, it is generally agreed that the researcherclinician who strives to evaluate an individual's speechreading performance in a reliable manner, is confronted with an extremely complex task. the connected discourse tracking (cdt) procedure one unique and highly versatile procedure which represents an alternative aproach to the measurement of speechreading skills in the hearing-impaired, is the cdt procedure, originally described by de filippo and scott (1978). tracking is a timed technique in which the talker (sender.) reads from a prepared text presented in a phrase-by-phrase manner. the receiver "listens" to each successive group of words as an entity, before repeating verbatim what the sender has said. if the receiver is correct, the talker proceeds to the next segment; if an error occurs in the repetition of the material, the talker and receiver employ various strategies to elicit a verbatim response. the goal of the procedure is to obtain a repetition that matches the text verbatim without the use of non-speechrelated cues such as gestures (de filippo & scott, 1978). tracking performance is quantified and scored in terms of the number of words transmitted per minute (wpm). this procedure can be differentiated from traditional approaches to speechreading testing on the following grounds: 1) it comprises a discourse task and therefore ongoing "meaningful speech" (and not isolated, single-word stimuli-) is employed. j 2 ) / l t can be described as interactive in nature. since the procedure utilises a dyad and the talker's behaviour depends on the speechreader's performance, the interactive dynamics of a real conversation are allowed to play an important role in the evaluation. 3) the scoring deviates drastically from conventional procedures in that the typically "all or nothing" approach to sentence scoring is not employed; rather, scoring results is a rate measure (although as a prerequisite a 100% accuracy is implied.) 4) the final wpm score relates to communication efficiency and, as such, represents a composite measure of communication rate. therefore, in contrast with traditional measures, the wpm score reflects all the components within the communication interaction, namely the transmission, processing and correction as well as response time. in fact, de filippo and scott (1978) have suggested that this task not only taps perceptual skills (and the accuracy thereof), but a combination of an individual's perceptual and communicative abilities. since the tracking technique's inception, it has been used with individuals of all ages and with a number of modifications for multiple purposes (owens &iraggio, 1987; osberger, johnson & miller, 1987). as mentioned previously, its most extensive use has been in studies documenting changes in communication efficiency as a result of sensory devices (cowan, alcanta, blarney & clark, 1988; de filippo & scott, 1978; sparks et al. 1979; muller, 1988). in addition, a number of studies [e.g. danz & binnie, 1983; lesner & kricos, 1987) have demonstrated its feasibility for intervention and training purposes. for the purpose of this article, discussions will be limited to the use of speech tracking with adults. appeal of t h e speech tracking procedure since tracking is a live-voice procedure, it can be easily administered without the use of any equipment. in addition, it allows for quantification of the efficacy of the communication process. the procedure requires a different passage to be used at each session and therefore any learning of the test material is prevented. in view of the fact that ongoing speech is used in an interactive manner, "real-life" conditions are more closely approximated with use of the cdt technique; the task is therefore known for its high face validity (levitt, waltzman, shapiro & cohen, 1986). as with many of the tracking method's predecessors, the development of this procedure seems to have arisen from clinical necessity. in retrospect, it is speculated that perhaps one of the major motivations for its development revolved around the aspect of repeated presentations. it seemed that the typical practice effects associated with perceptual tasks could be bypassed by means of this new technique. although difficulties associated with traditional speechreading tests have to some extent been overcome by the speech tracking procedure and while other problematic issues are shared by conventional methods of speech recognition testing, it has now become apparent that this new procedure has introduced different sources of variability, which have often been disregarded by the clinical and research communities (for example, hopkinson et al. 1986). these methodological issues appear to contra-indicate the use of tracking for purposes of evaluation and in research investigations employing an across-subject design(lesner, lynn & brainard, 1988). a recent position paper by tye-murray and tyler (1988) has been the first to review and identify formally the confounding variables specific to this technique. in addition, in order to minimize the shortcomings associated with the use of this test instrument, these authors have proposed stringent guidelines for within-subject testing. criticisms directed at continuous discourse tracking as a test procedure the overall concern regarding the use of tracking as a test instrument pertains to one major aspect, namely that the ultimate score achieved by a receiver will vary as a function of several extraneous variables. tye-murray & tyler (1988) maintain that these variables are almost impossible to control. as mentioned before, many of the problems associated with tracking as a test procedure are also relevant to the use of sentence-list speechreading tests. in their opinion, however, there are unique shortcomings specific to the use of the tracking task. this has led these authors to conclude that the speech tracking procedure "does not reliably measure a receiver's ability to recognize speech" (tye-murray & tyler, 1988, p. 227). in contrast de filippo (1988) has argued that "for evaluating speechreading performance tracking requires careful attention to the (very) same issues of assessment as any other die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafi· υ incomplete phrases v ^ α> false starts u. pauses v word-finding difficulties ^ others' polite forms i / ' reference to interlocutor v ' placeholders, fillers, stereotypes v / υ acknowledgements v <ο >-< h i self correction • c ,ts comment clauses hj κ sarcasm/humour hj κ control of direct speech indirect speech acts others vocal aspects: intensity v / pitch v / rate intonation v quality nonverbal aspects: facial expression head movement l / body posture breathing v/ social distance v gesture and pantomime • others i totals 1 5 8 16 13 , 2 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 claire penn, beulah sonnenberg and yael schnaier appendix β profile of communicative appropriateness name patient β features of sampling date unit of analysis person eliciting sample inapprop mostly inapprop some approp mostly j approp approp comments request 2 s reply • 5 * clarification request acknowledgement t / «85 teaching probe others topic initiation •β i topic adherence • ε ji g ο g topic shift 1 / ε -ji g ο g lexical choice •a a ^ idea completion c 3 idea sequencing others ellipsis tense use 1/ reference 1 / ο lexical substitution forms js δ relative clauses js δ prenominal adjectives conjunctions others interjections repetitions revisions t / _> c incomplete phrases s u. false starts 1/ pauses word-finding difficulties others polite torms reference to interlocutor 1/ placeholders, fillers, stereotypes υ to acknowledgements self correction l / 12 '35 o c comment clauses r sarcasm/humour control of direct speech / indirect speech acts / others / ' vocal aspects: intensity pitch rate a intonation 1 1 quality ο a c ? nonverbal aspects: facial expression v/ § i ζ ε head movement a body posture l/" breathing social distance • y gesture and pantomime others totals 15 2 11 7 0 1 0 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 25 language abilities of 18-month-old zulu speakers melissa a. bortz department of speech pathology and audiology university of the witwatersrand abstract the receptive, expressive and pragmatic language abilities of 18-month-old zulu speakers were assessed in order to obtain preliminary norms. twenty-five participants of the birth to ten cohort study were investigated using parent reports, mother-child and tester-child interactions. data was transcribed and analysed using nonparametric statistics. results demonstrated that receptively subjects understood two-part instructions. expressively, the mean lexicon was 4.12 words and mean length of utterance 0.65. pragmatically, subjects were functioning on a nonverbal level and exhibited culture-specific items. the results provided information which could enable speech, language and hearing therapists to engage in primary and secondary prevention. an appropriate test battery for these children is discussed. opsomming die respetiewe, ekspresiewe en pragmatiese taalvermoens van 18 maande oue zulusprekers isgeevalueer om voorlopige norms op te stel. vyf en twintig deelnemers aan die geboorte tot tien longtudinale studie is deur middel van oueronderhoude, moeder-kind en toetserkindinteraksies ondersoek. data isgetranskribeer engeanaliseer deur middel van nonparametiese statestiek. die resultate dui aan dat die proefpersone op reseptiewe vlak instruksies bestaande uit twee komponente, begryp het. op ekspressiewe vlak was diegemiddelde leksikon 4.12 woorde en die gemiddelde lengte van 'n uiting 0.65. op pragmatiese vlak het die proefpersone op 'n nie-verbale vlak funksioneer en kultuurspesifike gedrag vertoon. die resultate het inligting verskafwat spraak-taal-en-gehoorterapeute in staat kan stel om betrokke te raak in primere en sekondere voorkoming van taalprobleme. 'n geskikte toetsbattery vir hierdie kinders is bespreek. epidemiology is the professional discipline concerned with "searching put and understanding the factors relating to the occurrence of disease in the population" (peterson & thomas, 1978, p. xv). mausner and bahn (1985) cited by m. marge*1 (personal communication, april 20, 1992), have defined epidemiology as the study of the distribution and determinants of disorders in human populations. a cohort study is based upon one of the methods used by epidemiologists to study disease occurrence and has been described as "a systematic follow up of a group of people for a defined period of time" (the oxford reference concise medical dictionary, 1990 p. 142). cohen and manion (1991) have described cohort studies as prospective longitudinal methods which "are particularly appropriate in research on human growth and development" (p. 73). cohorts usually refer to a birth cohort which contain persons born in a specified period of time (miller & keane, 1983). although cohort studies belong to the realm of epidemiology, they are also a powerful tool that can be used for descriptive research, which is concerned with "acquisition of skills in young children" (cohen & manion, 1991, p. 70). this methodology has been adopted by speech, language and hearing therapists who have made use of cohorts for developmental research, such as the connecticut longitudinal study in which mother-infant communication was examined (thoman, 1981, m. marge (1992) professor communication sciences and disorders, division of special education and rehabilitation, syracuse university. p. 194). epidemiology, from a speech, language and hearing therapists' perspective, is concerned with the prevention of communicative disorders, where prevention usually occurs at three levels: primary, secondary and tertiary (gerber, 1990). primary prevention refers to the elimination of the occurrence of a communicative disorder, for example, by mass public education and the promotion of better health in general; secondary prevention focuses on early detection and treatment of a communication problem and tertiary prevention relates to rehabilitation, the traditional focus of attention for speech, language and hearing therapists (gerber, 1990; marge, 1991). marge (1984, cited in gerber, 1990, p. 319) has contended that "preventing communicative disorders should be the added new dimension to the professional responsibility of speech-language pathologists and audiologists". according to van haatum (1980, cited in gerber, 1990) this "will ultimately prove to be more productive than curing" (p. xiii). this has been reiterated by segal who quoted critical health, (1982,pp. 12-13) when motivating for a re-allocation of priorities: to direct more effort to prevention than cure. furthermore child and johnson (1992, p. 1) have declared "an ounce of prevention" may truly be worth "a pound of cure". the committee on prevention of speech-language and hearing problems of asha (1983) referred to in gerber (p. 311) strongly recommended that increased development, and implementation of primary prevention strategies should be undertaken, particularly for low-income populations, who are at the greatest suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 39, 1992 sasha 1 2 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 melissa a bortz risk for conditions that can lead to communication disorders. once early identification and screening for communication problems have taken place, it is possible to determine the incidence and prevalence of speech problems. in south africa, speech, language and hearing therapists have not concentrated their efforts on prevention of communication disorders, particularly for the "vast sections of the population who do not receive even the most basic speech therapy" (drew, 1982). a possible explanation for this is that we have limited tools available to perform the identification of speech and language problems, and those that do exist are inadequate (ballantine, ballantine & morgan, 1982), the writer, a speech, language and hearing therapist, was given the opportunity, to enter the field of epidemiology and prevention by participating in a cohort study entitled birth to ten: children of the nineties, which is currently in operation on the witwatersrand. this is a ten year longitudinal study, combining over thirty different disciplines studying the growth and child health development of children born between april and june 1990, in johannesburg and soweto. the speech and language project of birth to ten, is aimed at collecting information on the acquisition of speech and language in the targeted children, of various language groups prevalent in this area, with a view to establishing normative data. an additional aim of the study is to develop, modify and standardise measurement tools and procedures for assessing speech and language development. the purpose of this particular substudy was twofold: 1) to investigate the language abilities of 18-month-old zulu speakers with a view to establishing preliminary norms, and 2) to evaluate the effectiveness of the assessment materials utilised in the study. the establishment of such preliminary norms has "definite implications for secondary prevention, that is mass screening and early identification once the norms have been established" (m. marge, personal communication, april 20, 1992). it is necessary to study early language development so that timeous identification of language problems can be performed. this can prevent the broad and long term effects of language disorders in children, such as the "extreme emotional side effects of being learning disabled" (vorster, 1980, p. 2). the definitions of language and its components used in this study are as follows: language "is a code whereby ideas about the world are represented through a conventional system of arbitrary signals for communication" (lahey, 1988, p. 2). the components of language are receptive language, which is defined by nicolosi, harryman & kresheck (1989, p. 142) as "spoken messages received by the individual"; expressive language or the "use of conventional symbols to communicate one's perceptions, ideas, feelings or intentions to others" (nicolosi et al., 1989, p. 141); and pragmatics or use which is defined by bernstein & tiegerman (1989) as the rules relating to the use of language in social contexts. both the verbal and nonverbal aspects of pragmatics were considered and morphology, syntax and semantics were regarded both receptively and expressively. all these components are interrelated and interact dynamically in communication. bernstein & tiegerman (1989) have contended that from a developmental perspective, communication precedes and facilitates speech and language behaviour while both semantics ' the term toddler is used as an equivalent form of 18-month-old in this study. 2 zulu was spoken by 6,5 million people in 1980 according to the central statistical service. this is the most recent figure available. a new figure will only be available in late 1992 when the results of the 1991 census are published. and syntactic functions are derived from pragmatic experiences. from birth to 24 months children use different forms of behaviour such as vocal, gestural and lexical, to signal interaction or to produce speech acts, all of which occur in the social context. between 12 and 24 months children first acquire phonemes followed by lexical items based upon the emerging phonological and semantic systems (bernstein & tiegerman, 1989). 18-month-old children or toddlers1 can either be at the illocutionary stage of communication development, in that they use symbolic means to convey intentions, or at the locutionary phase in which they use language to express meaning (bates, camioni & volterra, 1979; ochs & schieffelin, 1979). gesell' (1954) maintained that 18-month-old children communicate by both gesture and words and, that in fact, words can be accompanied by gestures or even begin replacing these. the child may have a vocabulary of ten words and may be egocentric at this stage but it can be seen that she/he is beginning to communicate more than younger children. the 18-month-old plays independently but will react to companions. nicolosi & collins (1989) have claimed that 18-month-olds understand simple commands and prohibitions, recognise familiar objects and persons and identify one body part. expressively, they have an average sentence length of 1.5 words and say two or three word combinations. it was mentioned previously that the purpose of this study was to examine the language abilities of zulu speakers. the reason for this is that zulu is the most commonly spoken african language in south africa2. although it is the language primarily of natal and kwazulu, suzman (1990) has maintained that it is also the lingua franca of cities in the transvaal. linguistic features of zulu structurally, zulu is an agglutinating language in that various morphemes are combined to form a single word, in which simultaneously, each element maintains a distinct and fixed meaning (fromkin & rodman, 1978). it can also be described as a synthetic language in which grammatical relations depend mostly on affixes (bhatnager & whitaker, 1984); and a tonal language where tone has a grammatical function, a lexical function and serves to maintain syllable prominence (suzman, 1990). furthermore, zulu is a svo language with a reasonably flexible word order (suzman, 1990). the actual structure of zulu is characterised by three features: 'the noun class system, extensive concord and a full suffix system of verbal derivatives (doke, 1945). i taking the above mentioned information into account the present study was designed to determine the language abilities of 18-month-old zulu speakers by investigating the communication development from which language develops. a further goal was to devise a suitable repertoire of assessment procedures for this sample group. method subjects twenty-five first language zulu speakers from the birth to ten cohort study were selected. being participants in birth to ten implied that all subjects met the following criteria: subjects were residents in either johannesburg or soweto subjects had been enroled in the birth to ten from its inception, that is, they had antenatal, six month and one year questionnaires completed. the different disciplines participating in the birth to ten had asked pertinent questions at all of these stages which had been compiled into questhe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abilities of 18 month-old zulu speakers 27 tiormaires which were administered by specially trained fieldworkers. speech and language questions asked, included details about case history factors and biographical information such as languages spoken in the home. in addition for the purposes of this study all subjects met the criteria described below: all subjects resided in soweto, to ensure homogeneity of environment subjects were in the age range 18-20 months so that they could be expected to be both understanding simple commands and producing a small number of words and possibly joining these together in two word phrases (hopper & naremore, 1978; nicolosi&collins, 1989), thus providing sufficient communication behaviour to evaluate subjects were first language zulu speakers as reported by their parents in the birth to ten questionnaires 13 male and 12 female subjects were selected in an attempt to eliminate the effects of the sex variable all subjects were accompanied to the evaluation by the person who usually took care of the child, either the mother or the caregiver as "bonding with a caregiver... fosters speech development" (van riper & emerick, 1990, p. 92).3 procedures pretest preparation. all appointments for the assessment were made by research assistants who personally visited each subject and the caregiver, in soweto, in order to explain the purpose of the project and to give explicit directions on how to reach the test venue. these personal contacts proved successful as the response rate for attendance at the test situation was high. communication sampling procedures 1. parent report measure. according to dale (1991) parent report can provide valuable information on early child language development particularly in the age range 8-30 months. prior to the formal assessment research assistants asked mothers specific questions about the communication behaviours that their children were using. the parent report was devised by the writer based on information on development of child language from sources such as nicolosi & collins (1989); gesell (1954) and hopper & naremore (1978). (see 3 the term mother and caregiver will be used interchangeably. table 1: communication sampling procedures appendix a). 2. language sampling. a 40 minute sample of each subject's communicative behaviour was obtained, consisting of a 20 minute mother-child interaction, in which the mother and the child interacted freely, and a 20 minute tester-child interaction in which the clinician gave a variety of questions and commands in order to "analyse children's comprehension and expression through answers to questions and responses to commands" (miller, 1981, p. 13). this sampling procedure was loosely based on that described by miller (1981), when he suggested that 30 minutes should be used to obtain a language sample, as this is an appropriate length of time for the concentration span of 18-month-old children and that 30 60 utterances should be elicited in this period. all samples were recorded on a national vhs ms2 video camera and audio taped on a sanyo trc 2500 tape recorder, for back up. the testing situation took place in a one-way mirror room at the university of the witwatersrand speech and hearing clinic (ushc). 2.1 mother-child interaction. the objective of this interaction was to observe the communication between the mother and child. in order to stimulate communication, subjects and their mothers were given objects appropriate to age and culture, from two of the categories identified by reynolds (1989). stimuli therefore were either specific play objects eg. dolls and balls or items borrowed from the adult world such as eating utensils and towels. the stimuli were placed in accessible parts of the testing room and the caregivers were instructed to interact with their children as they would at home "in a natural manner" (wetherby, yonclas & bryan, 1989, p. 151). it is however, acknowledged that mothers may not play with, or speak to children in this way at home. 2.2 tester-child interaction. receptive and expressive language was elicited in the tester-child interaction by the child either having to choose or name an item from three familiar objects that is inkomishi 'cup', ibhola 'ball' and indishi 'plate' as for the reynell developmental language scales (1978): furthermore, pragmatic behaviour was elicited according to a "standard series of communicative situations" devised by wetherby and prutting (1984, p. 151) "to induce child-initiated communicative behaviour". the tester waited and looked expectantly at the child and responded naturally to the child's communicative behaviour. an example was to blow up a balloon, deflate it and then give it to the child and observe the reaction and to react appropriately to it. see table 1. ι receptive language abilities expressive language abilities pragmatic language abilities parent report questions 2, 4, 9 and 10 on appendix a, to elicit knowledge about receptive language questions 1, 3, 5 and 8 on appendix a to elicit knowledge about expressive language questions 6 and 7 to elicit knowledge about pragmatics mother-child interaction not tested mlu. mean number of utterances per turn. topic shifts qualitative assessment of communicative behaviours tester-child interaction child to identify inkomishi 'cup', ibhola 'ball' and indishi 'plate' and body parts child to name inkomishi 'cup', ibhola 'ball' and indishi 'plate' and body parts eliciting pragmatics from wetherby and prutting (1984) standard series of communication situations die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 melissa a bortz research assistants the study was conducted by four research assistants, diplomates in speech and hearing (community work) and who were fluent in both zulu and english. they established contact with the mothers, performed the testing and transcribed and scored the data. pilot study the aim of the pilot study was to determine the suitability of the procedures and stimuli described above and the length of time it would take to administer such a procedure. the pilot study was performed on seven subjects who met the criteria for the research, previously mentioned. a preliminary version of the research was devised, based on principles of language testing and materials devised for children of this age such as the reynell developmental language scales (1978). care was taken to make the procedures relevant and appropriate to the south african population so i consulted with researchers who had previously conducted investigations into zulu language acquisition such as s. m. suzman*2 (personal communication, october, 1991). in addition, preliminary scoring procedures were devised. results of the pilot study demonstrated that the proposed testing and scoring procedures were appropriate and provided useful information for analysis. furthermore, the pilot study indicated that the optimal length of time for the mother-child interaction was 20 minutes as children did not provide additional information for language sampling, thereafter. certain stimuli such as a teddy bear were eliminated as this frightened the subjects. analysis of the communication sample transcription. in pairs, the research assistants transcribed the mother-child interactions and the contexts in which these occurred, using standard orthography (miller, 1981; conti-ramsden & dykins, 1991). thereafter, pairs ofresearch assistants viewed segments of the video tape, repeatedly, until they established agreement on the interaction. in addition, the transcription was translated from zulu into english for me to understand what had been said. scoring. the following structural analysis was performed on the mother-child interaction. 1. mean length of utterance (mlu). the mlu for both child and mother was counted in morphemes because zulu has an "extremely rich morphology" (suzman, 1990 abstract) mlu was calculated according to miller's (1981) counting rules. paralinguistic features were not included and mlu was only counted for fully intelligible utterances. the mean number of morphemes and the mean number of utterances, used to calculate mlu, were judged individually, as these figures provide valuable information. percentage of clear and unintelligible utterances was also considered individually. 2. mean number of utterances per turn. the number of utterances for both child and mother were calculated in order to measure the density of each speaker's turn. this was determined by taking the number of utterances for each speaker, and the number of turns for that speaker and dividing them (g.conti-ramsden*3, personal communication, april 14 1992). *j s.m. suzman (1991) phd, department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand. * 3 g. conti-ramsden (1992), phd, centre for educational guidance and special needs, school of special education, university of manchester. 3. topic shifts. topic shifts occur when one of the conversational partners disengages her/himself from the previous set of concerns at either the verbal or nonverbal level (contiramsden & dykins, 1991). these were counted as having occurred when either the mother or the child changed both the focus and the theme of the conversation or the toys they were playing with as suggested by g. conti-ramsden (personal communication, april 14, 1992). 4. parent report and tester child interaction. the parent report and tester-child interaction samples were scored by a pair ofresearch assistants according to a format devised by the writer where responses were analysed as being correct or appropriate, incorrect or inappropriate, or, not tested. the 'not tested' category included any item not examined by the research assistants either due to a lack of response by the subjects or the subjects not responding to the previous stimulus eg. on the tester-child interaction if the subject could not point to indishi 'plate' because of loss of attention, then the next item, inkomishi 'cup', was not tested. the pragmatic interaction was scored as a communication act when "the child initiated interaction with the adult or focused attention on an object" (wetherbyetal., 1989, p. 151). wetherby etal. (1989) have described intentional communicative acts that occur as a result of the interaction in detail, but because the subjects only focused attention on objects, and there was little interaction with adults, this was not investigated further. all scores obtained were converted to percentages and analysed by means of nonparametric statistics. results will be discussed in terms of these values. interrater reliability research assistants worked in pairs in order to transcribe and score the full data sets. the, 30% of the data were randomly selected and independently recoded as suggested by contiramsden & dykins (1991). interrater reliability was calculated according to the following formula proposed by mcreynolds & kearns, (1983): percentage agreement agreements x i 0 0 agreements & disagreements attempts were made to achieve a minimum of 80% agreement with this formula but only 66% agreement was obtained. poor interrater reliability is not uncommon with children this age. bates, bretherton and snyder (1988) attempted to obtain similar agreement with 20 month olds and could not, as subjects of this age have "a relatively high proportion of unintelligible utterances" (p. 84). i results and discussion the results from the analyses of communication samples are presented below. they are divided into receptive, expressive and pragmatic language abilities, insofar as it was possible to separate these related skills. parent report, mother-child interaction and tester-child interaction will be discussed for each section. a discussion of a suitable repertoire of assessment tasks, based on these findings, follows. receptive language abilities parent report. from parents reports it appears that 95,5% of the subjects listened, when communication was addressed to them and responded appropriately to this communication for example, by either looking at or approaching the mother when called, while 4,5% of the subjects did not respond to communication. responding to communication is regarded as the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language abilities of 18 month-old zulu speakers onriate according to nicolosi & collins, (1989). all of age a pp™ p n w e r e reported to understand what was being said thechil r e ^ ^ ^ fouow two-part instructions such as hlalapant®1 «die 'sit down and eat' and hamba uyalale 'go ^ n i s o s i and collins (1989) have stated that 18-month-old h'ldren should be able to identify one body part. in this sam1 63 6% of children were reported to be able to do so while l ^ ' c o u l d not. children were able to identify ten different rts of their bodies and most often able to point to their izindlebe 'ears' (9 subjects), and least often to their ubuso 'face', iminwe 'finger' or isisu 'stomach' (see figure 1 for exact information). body part 29 0 2 4 6 8 frequency figure 1: frequency of correct identification of body parts children's ability to identify their body parts was also assessed in the tester-child interaction, in which it was found that ten of the subjects (40%) did not respond to this task while nine subjects (36%) could identify body parts such as izindlebe 'ears' and umlomo 'mouth' and six subjects (24%) could not identify parts of their bodies. the subjects' ability to identify body parts appears to be more advanced than their english speaking counterparts. hedrick, prather & tobin (1984), in their standardisation sample for the sequenced inventory of communication development, (sicd), found that children were able to point to their eyes, hair, mouth and nose at two years and only at 28 months to their ears, although certain subjects of this study could identify all the above, including ears, at 18 months. the results obtained in the parent-child report and the tester-child interaction were correlated using a spearman correlation coefficient resulting in a poor correlation (r=. 149). thus, parents maintained that children knew more parts of the body than the testing situation indicated. tester-child interaction. the majority of the subjects were able to point to the objects in the tester-child interaction: specifically 72% (18 subjects) were able to identify inkomishi 'cup', 80% (20 subjects) could recognise ibhola 'ball' and 60% (15 subjects) identified indishi 'plate' (see figure 2). it is interesting to note that fewer subjects were able to identify the objects as the procedure continued, while the percentage of children who could not be tested increased from 16% for the identification of inkomishi 'cup' to 40% at the end of the procedure, possibly signifying that the subjects were losing concentration towards the end of this task. to summarise the findings about receptive language abilifrequencles cup ball plate i yes sub no i i not t e s t e d figure 2: frequency for tester-child receptive abilities ties, it was found that 54% of children understood the questions, while 26% could not be tested in this way and 18% did not understand the instructions. therefore, the majority of children in the study were understanding symbolic representation and as such, relating words to toys (reynell, 1977). as regards the most suitable repertoire of assessment procedures to use for reception, the parent report appeared to yield more information than when the tester assessed the child, as it was difficult to obtain cooperation from children this young4. this confirms the findings of dale (1991) who maintained that parent report "is more representative of toddler language than laboratory samples" (p. 566). expressive language abilities parent report. nicolosi & collins (1989) have contended that jargon is at its peak at 18 months and gesell (1954) has reported that children of this age conduct expressive inflected "conversation" (p. 32). all subjects (100%) were reported to babble and use jargon. parents reported believing this to be appropriate in 74% of cases and inappropriate in 26% of instances. twenty-four subjects were reported as using imitation and all of the subjects were reported as able to sing, which is common in the expressive abilities of children this age. all mothers reported that their children were saying words. the subjects' words reported by the mothers are presented in figure 3. according to clark (1979, p. 160) the first words children say "show considerable agreement... across children and across languages". these first words refer to "here and now" topics and fall into the following categories: people, particularly "adult caretakers familiar to the child" food, houseword mama tata dipnltie matume " t i p let ha inla lbft?jg family names pnuza g o m β inkonilsi^ kudla dad a h a m o a 10 15 frequency 20 25 it should be noted that the reliability of parent report was not formally assessed. this is seen as a limitation of this study. figure 3: words children are saying according to caregivers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 hold items like cup, toys like ball, body parts and actions. it can be seen that the words the subjects were saying concur with these categories for example inkomishi 'cup', for household items. the mean lexicon of each child was 4,12 words, with a standard deviation of 2,2 words, and a pool of 25 words. dale (1990, p. 566) has asserted that parent report is "potentially an excellent measure of vocabulary development" and therefore these results could be regarded as providing information concerning a preliminary lexicon for these children, although it is acknowledged that for a truly representative lexicon a larger sample of children would have to be tested. the literature describes a broad range in the number of words that children are expected to produce at 18 months. this mostly depends on the source and particular discipline that is being referred to, as paediatricians tend to expect children to produce small numbers of words, for example illingworth (1983), has reported that children can name a single word at 18 months and holt (1977, p. 218) suggested that paediatricians should only be alerted to further investigation for an expressive language problem if "a child of 18 months" is not "producing at least one or two words". linguists and language therapists expect toddlers to be saying many more words at this stage, for example nicolosi & collins (1989) have noted that 18-month-olds produce 10 20 words while clark (1979) has stated that toddlers should have lexicons of approximately 50 words. it is interesting to note that, of the words the children were saying, several are not pure zulu forms but adoptives from other languages such asbhuti 'brother' and dankie 'thankyou' from afrikaans or no, from english. this is a common feature of the zulu that people on the witwatersrand speak. (m. mngadi*4, personal communication, july 20, 1992). in this sample, morphology was not formally assessed, but it was noted that some children used marked forms of the word and correctly denoted which noun classes the words belonged to, such as ibhola 'ball', noun class 5 and amanzi 'water', noun class 6 and others used the unmarked form manzi. (m. mngadi, personal communication, july 20,1992). this use of the prefix in zulu could be compared to english speakers' beginning to put two or three words together which is a normal development at this stage (hopper & naremore, 1978· nicolosi & collins, 1989). a more detailed investigation of morphology and syntax will be undertaken in a separate study in the birth to ten cohort programme. mother-child interaction. complexify measures of syntax revealed that the subjects had an mlu of 0,38 which is below the mean of 1,14 and 1,31, that english speaking children are reported as having at this stage (miller, 1981 ρ 27· bates et al., 1988, p.85). miller (1981, p. 25) has cautioned that "mlu can only be interpreted when the criteria for representativeness of the speech sample, such as sample size, have been satisfied". bates et al., (1988) have contended that to calculate mlu "a traditional minimum of fifty intelligible utterances" is required (p. 85). in the study of bates et al., (1988) investigating the mlu of 20 month olds, a mlu score of 1,00 was assigned to the children who produced only single-word utterances when "the number of intelligible utterances fell well below this criterion" (p. 85). even when using the same adjustments instituted by bates et al., (1988) for the ten subjects of this study whose number of utterances was below 50, the mlu for this sample remained below that of english speaking subjects, at 0,65. this mlu, however, indicates that the submelissa a bortz jects were functioning at the "early one-word stage" or " s e sorimotor stage v" which is acceptable in the age range 10 -ι"» months (miller, 1981, p. 55). bates et al. (1988) also found their subjects to have a rang of mlus of from 1,00 to 2,11. similar fluctuations were found in this study, with one subject not saying anything and another having a mlu of 1 word. it was found, however, that the children were using an average of 2,78 different morphemes i„ this interaction. the mean number of utterances produced was 72,4 with a large discrepancy between three in one child and 202 in another. the mean number of clear utterances for the sample was 1,17, while the mean number of unintelligible utterances was 0,74, indicating that although subjects was saying few utterances, most of those that were produced, were intelligible. it must be remembered, that, according to the rules for counting morphemes in each utterance, fillers such as 'aaaa' were not counted (miller, 1981, p. 24). these constituted a large portion of what the children were saying, as they were at the jargon stage of production (gesell, 1954). chapman, (1981, p. 206) has noted that "mothers speech averages about 2,4 morphemes longer than her child's during the 12-27 month period". results of the current study are not consistent with this finding, as mothers' mlu was approximately one morpheme longer than their childrens' (mothers mlu 1,4 and children's 0,65). a possible explanation for this is that mothers' mlu varies with conversational context and is shortest in free play situations, from which this sample was elicited (snow, 1972, cited in chapman, 1981, p. 206). the mlu for both mothers and children was more restricted in this sample, than the mlu reported in the literature for english speaking subjects. a pearson correlation coefficient indicated a poor correlation (r= 21) suggesting that the mothers' speech did not affect the childrens' speech (see figure 4). the mean number of utterances per turn for the children was 1,519 and for mothers, was 3,32, showing that mothers were saying more in each turn, than their children, which is to be expected given the superior language skills that adults have. there was a poor correlation between the mothers' speech and that of their children (r=.33). furthermore the subjects changed the topic more often than the caregivers with a mean number of 22 times and the caregivers with a mean of 18 times. this could suggest that subjects were concentrating on the conversation or toys that they were playing with, for short periods of tinie, which is to be expected at this age. 1200 1000 mother *4 m. mngadi (1992), senior tutor, department of african languages, university of the witwatersrand. figure 4: correlation between number of morphemes of mother and child tester-child interaction. the majority of subjects were not able to name items that they had identified in the receptive tasks, providing no response to items, so earning a "not tested" the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language abilities of 18 m o n t h d zulu speakers . q n t h e item indishi 'plate' three of the subjects classificatio_ ^ ^ ( 1 2 % ) ; one subject was able to name produced t a n ( j o n ] y four s u b j e c t s ( 1 6 % ) w e r e a b l e t q inkomism ^ f . a n d o n ] y four s u b j e c t s ( 1 6 0 / 6 ) w e r e a b l e t q inkofflis > o f the children was able to name name lbnoia u«ui • body parts. (see figure 5.) cup ball p l a t e i y e s i no w not t e s t e d figure 5: frequencies for tester-child expressive abilities from these results, it can be seen that the tester-child assessment of expression yielded very few examples of childrens' ability to name objects. these data indicating poor naming capabilities support the findings of the limited expressive abilities of these subjects on the mother-child interaction sampling. although the subjects exhibited restricted mlus in the mother-child test situation, their mlus (0,65) however, did indicate an ability to express themselves. these poor results therefore do not reflect an inability to verbalise, but, suggest the unsuitability of the task, for these children. the fact that the subjects' expression seems to be more limited than their reception is not unexpected, as it is well documented that the development of receptive language precedes expression. results of the tests of expression for these subjects did, however, indicate, that their expressive language abilities were more limited than those reported in studies performed on certain of their english speaking peers, as can be seen from the few words they are producing and their restricted mlu. pragmatic language abilities parent report. all the subjects were able to communicate their needs and 95,5% were able to do so appropriately. appropriate responses included saying mama or papa while pointing to the required object. inappropriate responses were those in which the parents did not understand the child's communication eg. in non specific crying. all children were also reported to be using gesture to communicate intention. mother-child interaction. an ethnographic approach as defined by mctear and conti-ramsden (1992) was taken for this analysis, where the emphasis was on qualitative, rather than on quantitative analysis eg. a description of all communicative behaviours was therefore recorded. the absence of evidence of a communicative behaviour however can not be construed as implying absence from a child's repertoire (roth & spekman, 1984). forty-four percent of the children attempted to sweep with a broom while 56% of subjects did not exhibit this behaviour. according to illingsworth (1983) who has commented that toddlers "copy mother in her domestic work for example sweeping the floor" (p. 145), this activity is typical. thirteen of the subjects were observed to carry dolls tied to 31 their backs, after the mothers had put them there. this is strongly related to how mothers traditionally carry their children (read, 1959). the above two activities are indicative of halliday's account of pretend play which is part of the imaginative act of phase i language functions and which occurred in her child at 18 months showing that these children are functioning in a similar way to their english speaking peers (halliday, 1975, cited in miller, 1981). in addition, subjects were observed engaging in autosymbolic play such as brushing their teeth with a toothbrush. they also used common objects and toys appropriately, such as playing with a car or ball. due to the qualitative nature of the analysis utilised for the spontaneous emergence of communicative behaviour, exact scores were not obtained but their presence demonstrates that the subjects were functioning at stage iii of westby's symbolic play scale check list, which occurs between 17 and 19 months (westby, 1980). it must be stressed however that non emergence of a behaviour was in no way interpreted as absence from the child's repertoire. researcher-child interaction. the results of the analysis of subjects' responses to the standard series of communication situations (wetherby & prutting 1984) are presented in figure 6. it can be seen that subjects responded positively to communication acts by focusing attention on them. communication a c t s w i n d u p toy s t a c k b l o c k s r e a d m a g a z i n e w r i t e w i t h p e n o p e n b u b b l e s inflate b a l l o o n blow windmill play x y l o p h o n e s p e a k o n p h o n e e a y t h a n k you indicate t h a n k s t a k e a w a y o b j e c t s53 a 10 16 2 0 26 i yes i no i i not t e s t e d figure 6: frequencies for tester-child pragmatic abilities saying thank you had more responses of not tested than positive responses: 52% of subjects were not tested on this item while 32% were able to thank. these results could be attributed to the fact that these targets could not be elicited in this way, as the subjects did not appear to be able to understand the verbal command to thank the tester. however, subjects were able to indicate thanks when they imitated their mothers doing so. the subjects' improved ability to indicate thanks could be culture specific as the thanking ritual is indicative of zulu culture where the subject is instructed papate 'thank' and then claps her/his hands before receiving something (gowlett, 1975, p. 14). the responses to the communicative acts were more often manifest by the child's focusing of attention on the object than by his/her interacting with the adult. according to wetherby et al., (1989), this is appropriate albeit on a nonverbal level. some vocalisation did occur and accompany gesture, for example during telephone play. the fact that children primarily responded to communication acts nonverbally corresponds with the limited level of the childrens expressive repertoire, previously discussed. this further demonstrates the related and integrated nature of the acquisition of language abilities. another possible reason for responses being mainly nonverbal could be attributed to the highly structured nature of the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 melisa a bortz task which did not, in fact, pragmatically, require a verbal response from the children: for example, there was no need to request or acknowledge others as described by coggins and carpenter (1978, in miller, 1981, p. 118). a further justification for responding to the communication act by focusing attention on the object, rather than initiating interaction with the parent, could be that children may not be used to playing with their parents, either because their parents are not at home due to work commitments, or because adults tend not to play with their children as play is regarded as being undignified. (reynolds, 1989). furthermore, few adults involve themselves in the play of children. play is not seen as sacrosanct and sometimes it is regarded with suspicion and as a waste of time. adults do not seek to direct play (reynolds, 1989, p. 87). it is also interesting to note that most of the objects used in this sample were not familiar to the children, except possibly the telephone, yet children responded appropriately to them. this is particularly relevant with obj ects that are not common in the homes, such as pens and paper as this has implications for school. general discussion in this study, some attempt was made to relate the subjects language to research performed on english speaking peers in order to investigate the possibility of common trends. attempts were also made to observe parallel trends in subjects whose language more closely resembles zulu, such as hebrew. there has been no report of these in the study as berman (1985, p. 267) has claimed that most of the developmental data obtained for hebrew "are biased in favour of the language of 2 3 year olds" and thus no trends could be observed. repertoire of assessment procedures. a comprehensive test battery, including parent report, mother-child and testerchild interaction was utilised in order to obtain these results. all measures were useful and complemented each other, for example, tester-child interaction was not found to be effective for eliciting expressive language, eg., naming body parts, yet this interaction provided worthwhile results for pragmatic, eg., trying to weigh up a toy, and receptive, eg., identifying body parts, language abilities. however, when correlating these interactions statistically, poor results were obtained indicating the need {jor such a comprehensive assessment at this stage of language development. nonetheless, although this test battery is believed to be extensive, it does not appear to be practical, as administration is time consuming and restricting in terms of requiring specialised and sophisticated equipment such as video cameras and specialist knowledge of language assessment and analysis. the results of this study, although indicating that all measures are useful, also indicate that parent report provides a general evaluation of all parameters of child language. thus, it is recommended that parent report beusedasa screening tool for children of this age, as this is cost effective; a valuable basis for a rapid evaluation of child language and a useful measurement over a wide range of social class (dale, bates, resnick & morriset, 1989; dale, 1991). parent reports are effectively used in appraisal at this stage of language development, for example, the receptive-expressive emergent language scale (bzochleague, 1979). in addition, parent reports are particularly suitable for the south african situation and for zulu-speakers specifically, because they have limited access to formal intervention facilities for speech, language and hearing disorders. parent reports can easily be utilised in the community by primary health care workers. the results of this study provide information about relevant content to include in such screening assessments. (see appendix b). the findings of this study have epidemiological implications on both a primary and secondary level of prevention. on a primary level, it is now possible to begin to educate people, particularly mothers and caregivers, about how to stimulate language at this level in order to prevent language problems from occurring. on a secondary level, this study has established preliminary norms for 18-month-old zulu speakers which were previously nonexistent. further research is required for validation and to establish more norms than these early findings so that speech, language and hearing therapists will be able to compare other children to these subjects in order to identify delayed or disordered language. once these norms have been established, mass screening of all zulu speaking children, of this age, can be undertaken in order to determine the prevalence and incidence of communication disorders. (m. marge, personal communication, april 20, 1992). such early identification would prevent the serious and long term consequences of language disorders that affect the psychological, educational and vocational dimensions of the child (bernstein & tiegerman, 1989; penn & segal, 1982, and aram & nation, 1980). conclusion the language abilities of these 18-month-old zulu speakers indicated that the development of the content, form and use of language are all integrated and that communication precedes and facilitates speech and language behaviour (lahey, 1988; bernstein & tiegerman, 1989). on a receptive level, subjects were able to understand two-part instructions, identify body parts and three objects. expressively, subjects were at the early one word linguistic stage, having a lexicon of 4,12 words and an mlu of 0,65, with a mean number of morphemes of 2,78 and a mean number of utterances per turn of 1,519. tester-child interaction provided few results as 85% of subjects could not name simple objects or body parts. pragmatically, the subjects responded mostly nonverbally, by focusing attention on an object rather than communicating verbally with the adult to ifems presented in communicative acts. this demonstrates the link between pragmatic and expressive language abilities and is to be expected in view of the limited expressive abilities. this analysis also contributed examples of pragmatic behaviour that were specific to zulu,culture, such as mothers tying a doll to their childrens' backs and the thanking ritual. as regards the second aim of this study, to evaluate the effectiveness of assessment materials, the previous discussion indicated that parent report provided a comprehensive method of assessing young children's language. in addition, this method is thought to be particularly suitable and useful for the south african situation, both in terms of the broad range of information it provides and in terms of being accessible to administration by a wide range of health personnel. this study has also established preliminary norms for 18-monthold zulu speakers which should be used to establish comprehensive norms to determine the prevalence and incidence of communication disorders in the zulu speaking population. as the year 2000 rapidly approaches, it is necessary that speech, language and hearing therapists make a concerted effort to undertake research of this nature such as incidence and prevalence studies, in order to provide an effective service to all people with communication problems in south africa. acknowledgements the author acknowledges with appreciation a grant from the hsrc, which funded this research. ursula booysen of the the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language abilities of 18 month-old zulu speakers 33 institute of biostatistics of the medical reseach council is sincerely thanked for her assistance and patience with the statistical data. furthermore, the immense contribution of bennedicta tlhomola, sylvia jongidiza, sydwell khene, bernice mdongoana and tebogo rankoane is acknowledged for their invaluable contribution in data collection and analysis. references aram, d.m., & nation, j.e. (1980). preschool language disorders and subsequent language and academic difficulties. journal of communication disorders, 13, 159-170. ballantine, j., ballantine, p.r. & morgan, r. (1982, july). auditory perceptual skills in zulu school children a preliminary normative investigation. sasha newsletter, 196, 5 20. bates, e., bretherton, i., & snyder, l. (1988). from first words to grammar: individual differences and dissociable mechanisms. cambridge: cambridge university press. bates, e., camaioni, l., & volterra, v. (1979). the acquisition of performatives prior to speech. in e. ochs, & β. b. schieffelin (eds.), developmental pragmatic, (pp.111-130). new york: academic press. berman, r.a. (1985). the acquisition of hebrew. in d. i. slobin (ed.), the crosdinguistic study of language acquisition vol. 1: the data (pp.255-371). hillsdale, new jersey: lawrence erlbaum associates. bernstein, d.k., & tiegerman, e. (1989). language and communication disorders in children (2nd ed.). columbus, oh: charles e. merrill. bhatnager, s., & whitaker, h.a. (1984). agrammatism on inflectional bound morphemes: a case history of a hindi speaking aphasic patient. cortex, 20, 295 301. bzoch, k.r., & league, r. (1979). receptive-expressive emergent language scale for the measurement of language skills in infancy. chapman, r.s. (1981). exploring children's communicative intents. in j.f. miller (ed.), assessing language production in children: experimental procedures, (pp. i l l 138). baltimore: university park press. child, d.r., & johnson, m.s. (1991). preventable and nonpreventable causes of voice disorders. seminars in speech and language, 12(1), 1-13. clark, e.v. (1979). building a vocabulary: words for objects, actions and relations. in p. fletcher & m. garman (eds.), language acquisition: studies in first language development (pp. 149-160). cambridge: cambridge university press. cohen, l., & manion, l. (1991). research methods in education (3rd ed.). london: routledge. ( conti-ramsden, g., & dykins, j.j(1991). mother-child interactions with language-impaired children and their siblings. britishjournal of disorders of communication! 26, 337-354. dale, p.s. (1991). the validity of a parent report measure of vocabulary and syntax at 24 months. journal of speech and hearing research, 34, 565-571. i dale, p.s., bates, e., reznick, j.s.,|& morisset, c. (1989). the validity of a parent report instrument of child language at 20 months. journal of child language, 16, 239-249. drew, m. (1982, september/october). editorial. sasha newsletter, 250, 1-6. i doke, c.m. (1945). textbook ofzulu grammar (4th ed.). london: longmans, green and co. fromkin, v. & rodman, r. (1978). an introduction to language (2nd ed.). new york: holt, reinehart and winston. gerber, s. (1990). prevention: the etiology of communicative disorders in children. englewood cliffs nj: prentice hall. gesell, a. (ed.). (1954). the first five years of life. a guide to the study of the preschool child. london: metheun. gowlett, d.f. (1975, november). toward black-white understanding in south africa. lecture delivered to the institute for the study of man in africa. johannesburg. , hedrick, d.l., prather, ε. m., & tobin, a. r. (1984). sequenced inventory of communication development. test manual, (rev. ed.). seattle: university of washington press. holt, k.s. (1977). developmental paediatrics: perspectives and practice. london: butterworths. hopper, r., & naremore, r.j. (1978). children's speech: a practical introduction to communication development (2nd ed.). new york: harper and row. illingworth, r. s. (1983). the normal child. some problems of the early years and their treatment (8th ed.). edinburgh: churchill livingstone. lahey, μ. (1988). language disorders and language development. new york: macmillan. marge, μ. (1991). introduction to the prevention and epidemiology of voice disorders. seminars in speech and language, 12(1), 49-73. mcreynolds, l.r., & kearns, k. p. (1983). single subject experimental designs in communicative disorders. baltimore: university park press. mctear, m.f. & conti-ramsden, g. (1992). pragmatic disability in children. london: whurr. miller, b. j., & keane, c.b. (1983). encyclopedia and dictionary of medicine, nursing and allied health. (3rd ed.). philadelphia: w.b. saunders. miller, j.f. (1981). assessing language production in children: experimental procedures. baltimore: university park press. nicolosi, l., & collins. (1989). developmental sequences of language behaviour: overview. in l. nicolosi, e. harryman, & j. kresheck (1989). terminology of communication disorders, speech-languagehearing. (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. nicolosi, l., harryman, e. & kresheck, j· (1989). terminology of communication disorders, speech-language hearing. (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. ochs, e., & schieffelin, b.b. (eds.). (1979). developmental pragmatics. (p. 110). new york: academic press. oxford reference concise medical dictionary. (1990).(3rded.). oxford: oxford university press. penn, c. & segal, d. (1982). case history factors in a group of languageimpaired children. the south afiicanjournal of communication disorders, 29, 3-14. peterson, d.r., & thomas, d.b. (1978). fundamentals of epidemiology. lexington, ma: d.c. heath and company. read, m. (1959). children and their fathers: growing up among the ngoni of nyasaland. fackenham: metheun. reynell, j. (1978). reynell developmental language scales, (rev. ed). london: national foundation for educational research. reynolds, p. (1989). childhood in crossroads: cognition and society in south africa. claremont: david phillip. roth, f. p., & spekman, n.j. (1984). assessing the pragmatic abilities of children: part 2. guidelines, considerations, and specific evaluation procedures. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 49, 12-17. segal, d. (1982, november/december). community-based therapy: some preliminary suggestions. sasha newsletter, 251, 15-18. suzman, s.m. (1990). language acquisition in zulu. unpublished doctoral dissertation. university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. thoman, e.b. (1981). affective communication as the prelude and context for language learning. in r.l. schiefelbusch, & d.d. bricker (eds.), early language: acquisition and intervention (pp. 181200). baltimore: university park press. van riper, c., & emerick, l. (1990). speech correction: an introduction to speech pathology and audiology (8th ed.). englewood cliffs, nj: prentice hall. vorster, j. (1980). manual for the test for oral language production. pretoria: s.a. institute for psychological and psychometric research, h.s.r.c. westby, c. e. (1980). assessment of cognitive and language abilites through play. language, speech and hearing services in schools, xi, 151-168. wetherby, a.m., & prutting, c. (1984). profiles of communicative abilities. journal of speech and hearing research, 27, 364-377. wetherby, a.m., yonclas, d.g., & bryan, a.a. (1989). communicative profiles of preschool children with handicaps: implications for early identification. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 54, 148 -158. address correspondence to ms m. bortz, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, private bag 3, wits 2050. 1 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 melisa a bortz appendix a questions used in the parent report [instruction: ask the mother or caregiver these questions the first time they come to wits.] 1) can your child say any words? yes/no. if yes, elaborate on the words. 2) does your child understand what you say to her/him? yes/ no. if yes: a) provide examples of what your child understands. b) provide examples of words you say that your child understands. 3) does it sound as if your child speaks to her/himself? yes/ no. if yes, what does your child say? [it should sound like they are speaking to themselves, but there may be no real speech, just the rhythm and intonation of speech. this is a clue or prompt that you can give to the mother if she doesn't understand the question.] 4) does your child listen to you when you call her/him or speak to her/ him? yes/no. if yes, how do you know this? 5) does your child copy what you say? yes/no. 6) does the child communicate when she/he wants something? yes/no. if yes, how does your child communicate? 7) does your child use her/his hands to communicate or point to what she/he wants? yes/no. 8) does the child sing? yes/no. 9) can your child understand and do simple instructions? yes/no eg. close the door, get the rag. if yes, what does your child understand, provide examples. 10) can your child point to or show parts of her/his body? yes/ no. if yes, which parts does your child know? based on nicolosi & collins, (1989); gesell (1954) and hopper & naremore (1978). appendix β receptive, expressive and pragmatic language abilities of 18-month-old zulu speakers receptive language listens and responds to communication addressed to him/ her understands two-part instructions, for example hlalapantsi udle 'sit down and eat' identifies a range of body parts identifies three objects expressive language babbles and uses jargon imitates and sings four word vocabulary inconsistent use of marked forms of noun classes mlu 0,65 words difficulty naming objects in a formal test situation pragmatic language demonstrates autosymbolic play such as drinking from a cup, washing with a face cloth uses common objects and toys appropriately for example pencil, paper, car and ball imitates parents in domestic tasks, for instance sweeping with a broom exhibits culture specific behaviours papate 'thanking ritual', carries doll tied to back vocalises, to gain adult's attention j gestures to get adult's attention ! simultaneously vocalises and gestures to achieve adult's attention , focuses attention on object, for example, attempts to blow up a balloon ! the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 87 an exploratory study of an undefined acquired neuromotor speech disorder within the context of the four level framework for speech sensorimotor control dunay schmulian, anita van der merwe and emily groenewald department of communication pathology university of pretoria abstract in this study, the speech of a 28-year-old male with acquired brain injury and who presents with an undefined neuromotor speech disorder which cannot be categorised as either apraxia of speech or dysarthria, is described. voice onset time, vowel duration, utterance duration and vowel formant analyses were done acoustically. a perceptual analysis and intelligibility rating were also executed. the subject was found to present with unique perceptual symptoms, intelligible speech, prolonged sound duration and distorted vowel quality. the results are interpreted within the context of the four level framework of speech sensorimotor control (van der merwe, 1997). opsomming in hierdie studie word die spraak van 'n 28-jarige manlike persoon met 'n verworwe hoofbesering, en met 'n ongedefmieerde neuromotoriese spraakafwyking wat nie as verbale apraksie of disartrie geklassifiseer kan word nie, beskryf. stemaanvangstyd, vokaalduur en uitingduur, sowel as vokaalformant-analises is akoesties uitgevoer. 'n perseptuele analise en verstaanbaarheidstoets is ook toegepas. die proefpersoon het gepresenteer met unieke perseptuele simptome, verstaanbare spraak, verlengde klankduur en distorsie in vokaalkwaliteit. die resultate word binne die konteks van die viervlak raamwerk van sensoriesmotoriese spraakkontrole geinterpreteer (van der merwe, 1997). key words: neuromotor speech disorders, four level framework. l b appreciate the speech production process and its overwhelming complexity, one only has to study the vast amount of literature attempting to capture the essence of this process and its concomitant pathologies. the identification of phases involved in this process remains problematic and yet a clear differentiation is necessary to comprehensively define sensorimotor speech disorders. mcneil and kent (1987) state that the assumptions underlying neurogenic pathologic populations need to be reconsidered. the reason being that there are existing pathologies that cannot be satisfactorily explained by the traditional conception of the speech production process. the origin of the traditional speech production model can be traced to the three stages involved in motor skill namely the encoding, programming and execution of body movements. this model was projected onto the speech production process and traditionally three stages have been distinguished: linguistic encoding, articulatory programming and execution of movements (darley, aronson & brown, 1975). aphasia is seen as a deficit on the highest level of the model namely linguistic encoding, apraxia of speech (aos) as an impairment on an articulatory programming level and dysarthria as a deficit on the level of execution of movement. reconsideration of our traditional conception, would question and evaluate the validity of the three level model. according to van der merwe (1997) a four level framework should be adopted on the grounds of differential neurophysiological involvement of the various brain structures involved in neuromotor function and that, therefore, a distinction should be drawn between motor planning and motor programming based on these neurophysiological grounds. the so-called association areas are believed to be responsible for motor planning (allen & tsukahara, 1974; brooks, 1986; marsden, 1984). the motor association areas, namely the premotor cortex (lateral area 6), the supplementary motor area (medial area 6) as well as the prefrontal and parietal association areas, are indicated in motor planning. brooks (1986) posits that the caudate circuit of the basal ganglia is part of the "higher" hierarchical level in the sense that it enables the high level plans to be translated into motor action. the neural areas involved in motor programming comprises the basal ganglia, the lateral cerebellum, the sma, the motor cortex and the fronto-limbic system. it is generally accepted that the basal ganglia and the lateral cerebellum in particular are involved in programming and these parts perform complementary functions. the exact role of each, however, is not known. there are indications that the basal ganglia has a more "sophisticated" role to play than the cerebellum. the motor cortex, the lower motor neurons, peripheral nerves and motor units are the neural structures involved in the execution phase of the hierarchy (van der merwe, 1997). thus differentiation between the functions of the neuromotor areas indicate involvement die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 88 dunay schmulian, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald on three motor levels, namely planning, programming and execution. speech production is also a motor act and it is probable that the same three motor phases can be distinguished. language planning is, however, also essential in speech. this implies an addition to the traditional three stage model of linguistic encoding, programming and execution of speech movements, resulting in a four level model / framework consisting of one pre-motor stage namely linguistic-symbolic planning and three motor stages namely motor planning, motor programming and the execution of movement (van der merwe, 1997). where the terms motor planning and motor programming were used interchangeably in the traditional speech production models (lapointe, 1982; itoh & sasanuma, 1984), van der merwe (1997) draws a clear distinction. motor planning is described as the recall of a core motor plan for each phoneme in the planned unit, sequential organization of the motor goals identified for each phoneme, adaptation of the core motor plans within the context of the planned unit and specification of motor plan subroutines. motor programming on the other hand, entails the selection and sequencing of motor programs for movements of the muscles of articulatory structures. specifications of muscle tone, movement direction, force, range, rate and the mechanical stiffness of joints required by the planned movement, are specified by the motor program (van der merwe, 1997). needless to say, a four stage model such as van der merwe's framework, holds vast implications for the classification of the different neuromotor speech disorders. it advocates a fourth grouping of disorders not yet identified or described clearly in the literature, namely a pure motor programming disorder. aos seems to fit the definition of a motor planning disorder, dysarthria due to basal ganglia, cerebellar and motor cortex lesions would represent coexisting problems in both motor programming and execution and lower motor neuron dysarthria indicates a disorder in the execution of movement (van der merwe, 1986; 1997; van der merwe, groenewald, brittz & grimbeek, 1995). the questions arise whether a pure motor programming speech disorder exists, and also what the symptomatology of such a disorder will entail. the above-mentioned definition of programming predicts that symptoms resulting from such a disorder would probably be sound distortions, defects in speech rate, or problems in the initiation of movement. these would occur in the absence of muscle tone disorders or involuntary movements which induce dysarthria (van der merwe, 1997). the differentiation between different speech disorders is not always clear cut. symptomatology may differ and yet if symptoms correspond, it does not necessarily imply or indicate a homologous problem. one way of addressing the essence of motor speech disorders is by manipulating certain contextual factors such as sound structure and the articulatory characteristics of the utterance and thereby placing differential demands on the motor system. by determining and comparing reactions to variation in speech context, more insight could be gained into the nature of motor speech disorders and it can contribute towards differentiating disorders in the different stages of the motor process (van der merwe, 1986). this same method is applied in the present study. if context sensitivity is displayed, the pattern of sensitivity can be compared to that of other subjects with neuromotor speech disorders who were studied in previous research (van der merwe, 1986; brittz, 1994; van der merwe, et al., 1995). the aim of this study is to describe the acoustic and perceptual symptoms and also the effect of variation of certain contextual factors on the speech of a person with an undefined acquired neuromotor speech disorder. based on the symptomatology, his speech disorder cannot be classified as either apraxia of speech, dysarthria or a combination of the two. we aim to describe the symptoms within the theoretical context of the four level framework (van der merwe, 1997), where it might be representative of an impairment on the level of motor programming in the speech production process. method goals and subgoals the main goal is to describe the perceptual and acoustic speech symptoms of a speaker with an acquired neurogenic speech disorder which does not fit the traditional classification of dysarthria or aos or a combination of both. the symptomatology will be explained within the theoretical context of the four level framework. subgoals • to identify symptoms occurring and to determine similarity with symptoms that are categorised under dysarthria or aos in world literature, through a perceptual analysis (perceptual analysis 1). • to describe certain temporal characteristics of the subject's speech through acoustic analysis of nonsense units that are systematically varied in sound structure and articulatory characteristics and to determine the possible reaction to manipulation of context (acoustic analysis 1). • to rate the intelligibility of words on a closed set format (perceptual analysis 2). • to analyze deviant vowel production as identified by the intelligibility test (acoustic analysis 2). research subject and listener panels research subject. the criteria for the research subject were: • the subject must display an acquired neuromotor speech disorder which does not fit the classification of either apraxia of speech or dysarthria or a combination of both, as diagnosed by an experienced speech pathologist specialising in the field of neuromotor speech disorders. the subject should not display the core symptoms of these disorders namely, effortful speech with self-initiated trials to correct errors, islands of error-free speech or sound substitutions and distorted sound substitutions as can be expected from aos or muscle tone abnormalities and involuntary movements which cause consistent distortion in dysarthria (mcneil, robin & schmidt, 1997: darley, et al., 1975; yorkston, beukelman & bell, 1988). • normal auditory comprehension and memory should be present to ensure comprehension of instructions. • reading skill must be adequate to read a passage orally. the selected research subject is a 28-year-old afrikaans speaking male. he has a head injury resulting from a motorcycle accident in 1989. he exhibited an indriven fracthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an exploratory study of an undefined acquired neuromotor speech disorder within the context g 9 of the four level framework for speech sensorimotor control the literature on aos and the different dysarthrias (kent ture (3cm by 3cm occipital). scan information further showed bleeding in the left motor cortex. he remained in a coma and on a respirator 7 weeks post-trauma. he presented with right hemiplegia and spasticity after regaining consciousness. he received intensive physiotherapy which helped to reduce the hemiplegia to near normal. he received intensive speech and language therapy at the speech-, language and hearing clinic at the university of pretoria for seven years post accident. the western aphasia battery (kertesz, 1982) was performed yearly and overall scores stabilised and formed a plateau from 1991 onwards. he obtained an aphasic quotient of 94.2 classifying him as a mild anomic aphasic. the pragmatic protocol (prutting & kirschner, 1983) also showed fewer inappropriate behaviours since 1992. speech intelligibility was assessed with the "afrikaanse toets vir spraakverstaanbaarheid" (klopper, 1983) and scores prior to dismissal were respectively 83% and 91% for intelligibility on single words and sentences. other assessments include screening for closed head injury as well as tests identifying and rating severity of dysarthria (van der merwe, 1985) and aos (van der merwe, 1986). his speech could not be classified as either purely apraxic or dysarthric or a combination of the two. listener panels two listener panels were utilized in the study. the first was a sophisticated panel which did the perceptual analysis of characteristic symptoms as this required specialised skills and a semi-sophisticated panel which did the intelligibility scoring as these scores would be representative of intelligibility for the general population. the criteria for the sophisticated listener panel stipulated that members should be experienced speech language pathologists specialising in the field of neuromotor speech disorders and the perceptual analysis of neuromotor speech disorders. the selected panel consisted of two speech pathologists as the analyses were done by consensus. the criteria for the semi-sophisticated listener panel stipulated that the members should be afrikaans-speaking and have knowledge of normal and abnormal speech production. the selected panel consisted of six final year communication pathology students at material the university of pretoria. perceptual analysis: the symptom profile a list of possible symptoms indicative of neuromotor speech disorders was compiled, using symptoms listed in & rosenbek, 1983; yorkston, et al., 1988; mcneil, et al., 1997, darley, et al., 1975). the sophisticated listener panel indicated presence or absence of the symptom in the subject's tape recorded speech. the subject was recorded while reading a phonetically balanced passage ("in die wildtuin") which was developed for research purposes. acoustic analysis 1: voice-onset-time, vouiel duration and utterance duration the material for this analysis was duplicated from a similar, but more comprehensive study by van der merwe (1986) in which the influence of certain contextual factors on temporal features were studied. the material consists of groups of nonsense syllables that differ in sound structure and articulation characteristics. five sound structure groups and four articulation characteristic groups can be distinguished. the sound structure groups differ in length, sequence of consonants (c) and vowels (v), as well as the number of phonemes in every utterance. the articulation groups differ in place and manner of articulation. nonsense syllables make the strict control of sound structure and articulation characteristics possible. the material consisted of 20 nonsense units. each unit was repeated six times and the five most constant repetitions were analysed. table 1 contains the nonsense units in their respective articulation characteristics and sound structure groups. perceptual analysis 2: intelligibility test in order to ascertain intelligibility on a single word level, a list of twenty words from the "afrikaanse verstaanbaarheidstoets vir disartriese sprekers" (klopper, 1983) were applied. listeners had to select and circle the appropriate word out of a closed set of nine similar sounding words. acoustic analysis 2: formant values in order to ascertain differences from normal formant values of vowels, as obtained from a previous study (van der merwe, groenewald, van aardt, tesner & grimbeek, 1993), the two most deviant vowels (as determined during perceptual analysis 2), namely [a] and [a], were analysed. the vowels were taken from the phonetically balanced passage which the subject read. the contexts in which these vowels occurred, were controlled by analyzing identical words or vowels with similar phonetic contexts. table 1: nonsense units slotted into sound structure and articulation characteristics sound structure articulation characteristics 1 1.1 babe 1.2 bobs 1.3 dade 1.4 dode 2 2.1 bats 2.2 bats 2.3 dake 2.4 doks 3 3.1 batef 3.2 botsf 3.3 dakef 3.4 doksf 4 4.1 bat 4.2 bat 4.3 dak 4.4 dak 5 i 5.1 batefup 5.2 botefup 5.3 daksfup 5.4 daksfup die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 90 dunay schmulian, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald apparatus recording equipment for acoustic analysis 1 and 2: akg d1200 ε short distance, directional microphone, a nakamichi versatile cassette system and a tdk 90 minute magnetic cassette. analysis equipment for acoustic analysis 1: a nakamichi 550 cassette was used to send the speech signal to the kay dsp sona-graph model 5500 where the speech signal was analysed by a digital signal processor. the speech was monitored with two jbl pro 3 loudspeakers. the speech signal was displayed on a nec multisync 2 display screen, where the time cursors and a time axis were used to obtain all required measurements, l b permit comparison to controls used by brittz (1994), it was decided to use the identical analysis set-up and recording equipment. analysis equipment for acoustic analysis 2: the computer speech laboratory (csl) model 4300 β of kay elemetrics corporation was used because of its automatic formant tracing facility. procedure as it was an exploratory study, it was decided to commence the study by allowing the sophisticated listener panel to listen to a tape recorded speech sample of the subject to determine the diagnostic, neurogenic speech disorder category of the subject (perceptual analysis 1) by identifying the symptoms in his speech. it was then decided to study the influence of variation in contextual factors, on the three temporal features previously mentioned, and to determine whether context sensitivity occurred (acoustic analysis 1). the data of normal control speakers studied by van der merwe, et al. (1993) and brittz (1994) were used for comparative purposes. a semi-sophisticated listener panel's intelligibility rating and subjective diagnostic interpretation of the subject's speech was obtained. it was also required of this panel to identify sounds perceived as deviant from normal production (perceptual analysis 2). the analysis of the formants of the subject's vowels [a] and [d], was executed as a result of the semi-sophisticated listener panel's perception that vowels were more deviant from normal production than consonants, particularly [a] and [o] (perceptual analysis 2). data analysis perceptual analysis 1: symptom profile symptoms of neuromotor speech disorders, as taken from the literature, were tabulated to form the starting point from where to examine all other results. the presenting symptoms were identified by applying the method of analysis through consensus of two sophisticated listeners. acoustic analyses training in the use of the analysis equipment, and the technique involved in acoustic measurements, was received from a researcher who had obtained a masters degree in the field and who is an experienced user of the equipment. the analysis was done by the first author and problematic analyses received consultation from the above-mentioned researcher. acoustic analysis 1: voice-onset-time, vowel duration and utterance duration three temporal speech parameters, namely voice-onsettime (vot), vowel duration (vd) and utterance duration (ud) were measured. identical analysis set-ups were used for all three speech parameters. a twofold analysis of the speech signal was displayed simultaneously on the screen, with the sound wave on the upper half of the screen and a broadband spectrogram of the speech sound energy between 0 and 8000 hz on the lower screen. with vot, vd and ud, the two representations were constantly compared to increase reliability of the measures. vot of the initial voiced plosives was measured. the initial consonant was selected for analysis as it indicates more sensitivity towards contextual factors (ingrisano & weismer, 1979). the vot is measured from the plosive to the commencement of the first vertical striate on the spectrogram that represents glottal pulsing (lisker & abrahamson, 1964). the commencement of the plosive was regarded as the graphic nil point and the voice onset after the plosive obtained a positive notation, while the voice onset preceding the plosive was noted as negative. the range of vot was calculated for each category, in other words, the biggest negative, or the smallest positive value was recorded as borders of the range. the criteria for normal vot, namely -180 msec to +15 msec (van der merwe, 1986; brittz, 1994) was applied to the range of vot. the speech parameter vd was measured during the production of a short vowel after the initial plosive. the transition between a consonant and the following vowel provides a context for the greatest variance in acoustic parameters (gay, 1979). the speech parameter ud was measured as the first cvcv unit of each utterance and cvc in the case of structure 4 units. measurement started from the commencement of the first vowel, i.e., the plosive where a positive value was obtained, or voice onset in cases of negative vot values. the processing of vd and ud were both conducted as follows: * the median values of the five repetitions of the nonsense syllables were calculated using non-'parametric statistical analysis in order to make comparison with the, control data possible. the control data was taken from a study by brittz (1994) who used the same material and did the study in the same laboratory. the control subject used in that study was a normal male speaker in the same age group as the subject used in the current study. * the influence of variation of the contextual factors was examined to determine if the subject showed context sensitivity. acoustic analysis 2: vowel formants the speech signal was received from the cassette tape and captured at a sampling rate of 10 000 hz (i.e. 0-4062 hz) for analysis. the steady state of the vowel was marked with time cursors. a wideband spectrogram, as well as a linear prediction coding (lpc) formant history was obtained of the captured steady states of the respective productions of [a] and [o]. the mean value of formant 1, 2 and 3 was obtained by means of the formant history statistics provided by the csl. the absolute formant values (mean value) of formant 1, 2 and 3 of the respective productions of [a] and [o] were the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) exploratory study of an undefined acquired neuromotor speech disorder within the context g i of the four level framework for speech sensorimotor control and delineate this unusual speech disorder, the speech c o m p a r e d to the control data of van der merwe, et al. (1993) using non-parametric statistical analysis. in the study of van der merwe, et al., (1993) the formant patterns of the vowels of afrikaans were studied. ten males aged between 18 and 45 years, with no speech deficits and mother tongue speakers of afrikaans, were selected. formant frequency values of the same order were obtained from the ten subjects for all vowels. the overall conclusion was that reliable data was obtained. the relative formant relationships of f1/2, f2/3 and fl/ 3 were also calculated and compared to the formant relationships found in normal speakers (van der merwe et al., 1993) using non-parametric statistical analysis to determine whether the current subject's formant relationships fall within normal intervals. perceptual analysis 2: intelligibility test each panel member's score of the number of words correctly identified was individually calculated out of 20 and converted to a percentage. the type of error was also noted. results findings of the sophisticated listener panel regarding symptom profile in order to determine if the subject belongs to either the diagnostic category of aos or dysarthria, a sophisticated listener panel listened to a tape recorded sample of continuous speech. the finding was that, although highly intelligible, the subject could not be classified on the basis of salient perceptual features as either verbally apraxic or dysarthria the subject displays symptoms from both diagnostic categories (see table 2), but in a unique combination. variable, often normal rate with inconsistent prolongations of phonemes occurred, together with inconsistent distortion in the spatial and temporal dimensions of utterances, particularly vowels, which appeared to be due to articulatory telescoping. the subject's speech cannot be categorised as the slow, struggling speech of an apraxic speaker, yet does not exhibit the constant distorted quality of dysarthric speech. the conclusion was thatjthe subject belongs to a unique category of neurogenic speech disorders. in order to describe symptoms were further analyzed. acoustic analysis 1 voice-onset-time (vot) vot was examined by categorising the articulation groups into the five sound structure groups hereby determining whether context sensitivity was present in such a controlled sound structure environment and if such a controlled environment had any influence on the presence and possible type of error presented by the subject. the range of vot scores are given in table 3. the subject's vot ranges (smallest and biggest vot measure of each unit) are compared to the normal range of-180 msec to +15 msec. in the category si, three of the four articulation characteristic groups' vot scores deviated from the normal range: a l by 45 msec, and a3 and a4 both by 2 msec. in the category s2, two articulation characteristic groups' vot deviated from the norm (a2 and a3), both with 2 msec. in sound structure categories s3, s4 and s5, all vot scores of the articulation groups fell within normal limits. fifteen of the twenty vot scores ranged from -180 to + 15 msec, which is within normal limits. this represents-normal vot scores in 65% of all nonsense unit utterances. four of the five errors are so marginal that they cannot be depicted as true vot errors which leaves one error. it can be explained by the fact that it was the first utterance produced by the subject and he could possibly have been rather anxious to articulate to the best of his ability. the data seem to indicate that the subject rarely produces vot's outside the normal range and also that those errors that did occur are to a certain extent context sensitive for variation in sound structure. due to the low frequency of errors, it is not possible to make any final conclusions in this regard. vowel duration (vd) the vd of the subject is displayed with articulation characteristics and sound structure groups in table 4. the general tendency seems to be longer than normal vd, with occasional normal vd and only one occurrence of shorter than normal vd. because of only one shorter vd table 2: salient features of aos and dysarthria with the presence and absence of feature in speech of the apraxia of speech subject dysarthria subject slow struggling speech absent slow speaking rate absent slow speaking rate absent variable rate present inconsistent distortion present consistent distortion absent spatial and temporal distortion present short rushes of speech absent normal respiration and phonation present respiration and phonation problems absent trial and error articulation absent phonemes prolonged present false starts and restarts absent inappropriate silences absent prolongations present monoloudness absent syllabic speech absent articulatory telescoping present die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 92 dunay schmulian, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald and inconsistent normal durations, no conclusions can be drawn regarding context sensitivity. the longer vd scores do not show any tendencies regarding either sound structure nor articulation characteristics. longer vd occurred in all sound structure groups across all articulatory groups with the exception of sl/al, s2/a4, s5/a1 and s5/a4 which showed shorter, longer, normal and normal vd respectively. the frequent longer vd that occurred does not seem to be context sensitive as no clear pattern emerged from the results. statistical analysis indicated that vd exceeded normal duration significantly. utterance duration (ud) the ud of the subject is displayed with sound structure and articulation characteristics in table 5. the general tendency seemed to be toward longer than normal ud, with normal and shorter than normal ud occurring occasionally. longer ud was represented in all sound structure groups across all articulation characteristable 3: vot results with ranges indicated sound structure articul. group unit range of vot (msec) normal/ deviant si a l babe -184,4 +56,25 deviant with 45 msec a2 bobe +7,81 +10.94 normal a3 dads -37,5 +17,19 deviant with 2 msec a4 dode +10,94-+17,14 deviant with 2 msec s2 a l bate +6,25 +14,06 normal a2 bote +9,37 +17,19 deviant with 2 msec a3 dake +10,94 +17,19 deviant with 2 msec a4 doke +4,68 +10,94 normal s3 a l batef +3,25 +10,94 normal a2 botef +10,94 +12,50 normal a3 dakef +10,94 +15,63 normal a4 dokef +9,37 +15,63 normal 1 s4 a l bat +7,81 +10,94 normal a2 bot +4,68 +7,81 normal a3 dak +12,50 +14,06 normal a4 dok +10,94 +15,63 normal s5 a l batefup 0 +15,63 normal a2 botefup +7,81 +12,50 normal a3 dakefup +10,94 +14,06 normal a4 dokefup +10,94 +15,63 normal the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 tic groups except with s3/a1 and a2 (shorter than normal ud) and s4/a1, a2, a3 (normal ud was recorded). this observation was confirmed by statistical analysis. shorter than normal and normal ud did occur, but it occurred inconsistently. due to the small sample of analysis material of temporal speech characteristics, such as ud, it is not possible to make final conclusions in this regard. findings of the intelligibility ratings intelligibility scores are displayed in table 6. the average intelligibility was calculated at 92,5% with scores of 80%, 85%, 95%, 100% and 100%. only nine words were perceived incorrectly out of 100 evaluations overall (20 words produced and evaluated by 5 panel members). interrater comparisons revealed little differences between the intelligibility scores. it was therefore ascertained that the subject was highly intelligible. the phonetic nature of the material aided description of errors. two types of errors occurred: r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) exploratory study of an undefined acquired neuromotor speech disorder within the context g 3 a " t h e p o u r level framework for speech sensorimotor control table 4: vowel duration results with contextual factors sound structure and articulation characteristics ίππι^ι·"-" sound structure articul. group unit vd of control vd of subject (in msec) comparison using non-parametric statistical analysis si ' a l babe 136 148 153 153 118.7 114.1 85.9 171.9 35.9 shorter a2 bobe 116 111 117 117 135.9 125 132.8 135.9 129.7 longer a3 dade 140 144 144 159 159.4 184.4 190.6 198.4 190.6 longer — a4 dode 123 121 119 120 148.4 159.4 159.4 148.4 178.1 longer s2 a l bate 116 123 123 123 139.1 129.7 120.3 123.4 134.4 longer a2 bote 109 112 121 116 126.6 115.6 139.1 134.4 107.8 normal a3 dake 106 112 119 120 139.1 132.8 132.8 121.9 123.4 longer a4 doke 100 112 119 92 151.6 135.9 134.4 132.8 134.4 longer s3 a l batef 114 107 112 116 117.2 121.9 125 109.4 131.2 longer a2 botef 116 112 102 98 135.9 153.1 173.1 151.6 162.5 longer a3 dakef 111 111 111 114 134.4 123.4 117.2 126.6 121.9 longer a4 dokef 98 112 112 101 140.6 140.6 140.6 153.1 135.9 longer s4 a l bat 144 150 150 139 150 168.7 156.3 153.1 157.8 longer a2 bot 136 133 140 125 168.7 173.4 160.9 164.1 168.7 longer a3 dak 112 109 128 128 190.6 185.9 170.3 168.7 182.8 longer a4 dok 125 127 123 128 181.2 190.6 182.8 181.2 178.1 longer s5 a l batefup 122 122 117 127 118.7 128.1 109.4 109.4 106.2 normal a2 botefup 106 89 92 100 134.4 134.4 148.4 170.3 139.1 longer a3 dakefup 103 120 107 103 132.8 137.5 132.8 115.6 123.4 longer a4 dokef up 108 109 97 105 106.2 129.7 110.9 101.6 121.9 normal table 5: utterance duration results with contextual factors sound structure and articulation characteristics indicated . sound structure articul. group unit vd of control vd of subject (in msec) comparison using non-parametric statistical analysis si a l babe 512.5 523 589 564 764.1 754.7 564.1 643.7 700 longer a2 ! bobe 562 717 728 525 639.1 723.4 498.4 701.6 687.5 longer a3 i dade 344 397 428 464 681.2 670.9 821.9 771.9 798.4 longer a4 1 dode 428 448 455 433 712.5 764.1 754.7 754.7 750 longer s2 a l j bate 447 472 575 584 698.4 704.7 687.5 689.1 701.6 longer a2 bote 412.5 414 527 528 662.5 695.3 701.6 712.5 570.3 longer a3 dake 317 420 409 450 623.4 632.8 659.4 645.3 682.8 longer a4 doke 395 589 553 592 612.5 598.4 614.1 626.6 631.2 longer s3 a l batef 534 547 561 616 482.8 484.4 498.4 498.4 451.6 shorter a2 botef 527 561 570 494 504.7 512.5 493.7 523.4 510.9 shorter a3 dakef 350 417 402 422 528.1 548.4 546.9 529.7 506.2 longer a4 dokef 409 408 489 425 495.3 520.3 523.4 537.5 526.6 longer s4 a l bat 312:5 320 484 325 379.7 410.9 379.7 351.6 398.4 normal a2 bot 327 502 494 325 404.7 403.1 390.6 406.3 406.3 normal a3 dak 272 411 319 406 404.7 398.4 367.2 387.5 392.2 normal a4 dok 320 366 306 319 418.7 421.9 403.1 375 365.6 normal s5 a l batefup 464 494 436 478 381.2 364.1 331.2 364.1 373.4 shorter a2 botefup 333 451 417 350 456.2 493.7 476.6 475 484.4 longer a3 ' dakefup 452 334 334 353 426.6 437.5 454.7 443.7 434.4 longer a4 dokefup 353 353 344 314 403.1 421.9 404.7 415.6 418.7 longer die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 94 dunay schmulian, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald * continuants were perceived as plosives in 55% of all errors. * initial devoiced consonants were perceived as voiced in 44% of the occurring errors. although the errors give some insight into the phonetic nature of the problem, the error scores are marginally small. the semi-sophisticated listener panel members, who had no background information on the case, perceived the subject as a second language speaker or a clutterer when they were asked for a subjective diagnostic interpretation of his speech. they perceived his vowels to be more deviant from normal production than his consonants and the [a] and [a] as particularly deviant. formant analysis of distorted vowels the decision to analyze vowels was based on both the sophisticated and the semi-sophisticated listener panels' observation that the subject's production is deviant from table 6: scores obtained by the semi-sophisticated listener panel in the intelligibility rating listener panel panel member (lpm) score out of 20 percentage lpm 1 16 80% lpm 2 17 85% lpm 3 19 95% lpm 4 19 95% lpm 5 20 100% lpm 6 20 100% normal afrikaans mother tongue speakers' vowel production and more so than his consonant production. the vowels identified as being most deviant were the [a] and [a]. the first three formant frequencies of [a] were analyzed on six productions and [a] on five productions of the sound in similar contexts. the average formant frequency values of the current subject's production of [a] and [a] are displayed in table 7 and 8 respectively. the data obtained is characteristic of the subject's [a] and [a] production in continuous speech. a comparison of the average formant frequency values of van der merwe et al. (1993) (the control group) and the current subject is displayed in table 9 for [a] and in table 10 for [a]. in the selected productions of [a] by the subject (see table 7 and 9), the following was observed:• the formant frequency values of formant 1 (fl) lie between 515.4 hz and 760.3 hz with an average value of 638.4 hz. the control group's values for formant 1 of [a] range from 521.7 hz to 778.7 hz with the average value for formant frequency 1 calculated at 697.2 hz. using non-parametric analysis, no significant difference between the subject and the control data could be found. although the subject's fl falls within normal limits with non-parametric statistical analysis, there is some variability in the respective values obtained. for productions of aan, the values varied between 744.2 hz and 760.3 hz (a difference of 16 hz). in aandete, the value lowered to 515.4 hz which is more than 200 hz lower. • the formant frequency values of formant 2 (f2) of the subject lie between 1260.1 hz and 1571.6 hz with the average frequency value for f2 of [a] calculated at 1421.2 hz. the frequency values for formant 2 of the control group range from 924.1 hz and 1295.6 hz with the average calculated at 1113.7 hz. according to non-parametric statistical analysis, the subject's mean for f2 was significantly higher than the median of the control group. the variability in formant values continued in f2 with aan land aan 2 obtaining values of 1571.6 and 1443.2 table 7: the formant frequencies of six productions of [a] in similar phonetic contexts formant frequency in hz for six productions ; formants aandete aan 1 aan 2 gaan daar 1 1 daar 2 | 1 formant 1 515.4 744.2 760.3 539.6 655.9 615.1 1 1 formant 2 1260.1 1571.6 1443.2 1559.6 1377.1 1315.7 ' formant 3 2564.0 2872.4 2680.9 2840.0 2575.0 2479.8 formant frequency in hz for five productions formants samekoms kom word rok ongemaklik formant 1 480.7 478.8 503.9 662.6 585.8 formant 2 1982.9 1738.9 1006.7 1557.6 1975.2 formant 3 3314.3 3027.3 2472.0 2662.7 2825.0 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 table 8: the formant frequencies of five productions of [ ] in similar phonetic contexts r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an exploratory study of an undefined acquired neuromotor speech disorder within the context of the four level framework for speech sensorimotor control 95 hz respectively a difference of 128 hz. there is a difference of 61 hz between values obtained for daar. • the formant frequency values of formant 3 (f3) of the subject's production of [a] lie between 2564.0 hz and 2872.5 hz with the average calculated at 2668.2 hz. in the control group, formant 3 values of [a] range from 2213.6 hz and 2798.1 hz with the average calculated at 2479.8 hz. non-parametric statistical analysis showed that the median of the subject was significantly higher. f3 showed more variability in values obtained for aan 1 and aan 2 : 2872.4 hz and 2680.0 hz respectively. the difference between those two calculate to almost 200 hz. the value of daar 1 (2575 hz) and daar 2 (2479.8 hz) differed by 95 hz. in the selected productions of [a], (see table 8 and 10) the following were observed:• the formant frequency values obtained for formant 1 (fl) of the subject, lie between 478.9 hz and 662.6 hz with the average calculated at 542.3 hz. in the control group, formant frequency values for fl range from 298.8 hz to 433.6 hz with the average calculated at 373.8 hz. no identical words could be used for formant analysis of [aj. three of the five productions contained [a] followed by a nasal sound : samekoms, kom, ongemaklik with their fl values of [a] respectively 480.7, 478.8 and 585.8 hz. even though samekoms and kom differed in number of syllables, their formant values of [a] differ with just more than 2 hz. however, f l of [a] in ongemaklik differ from them both by almost 100 hz. • formant 2 (f2) values of the subject lie between 1006.8 hz and 1982.9 hz with the average calculated at 1652.3 hz. in the control group, values of f2 of [a] range from 708.2 hz to 942.8 hz with the average calculated at 805.6 hz. f2 of [a] followed by a nasal sound were 1982.9 hz (samekoms), 1738.9 hz (kom) and 1975.2 hz (ongemaklik). the f2 values for production of samekoms and ongemaklik, the three syllabic words, were closer and both of them differed considerably from the value of [a]1 in kom at 1738.9 hz, 200 hz below their value. j • the formant frequency values of formant 3 of [a] of the subject lie between 2472.0 hz and 3314.3 hz with the average calculated at 2860.2 hz. in the control group, formant frequency valuesjrange from 1991.2 hz to 2754.4 hz with the average calculated at 2333.3 hz. the values of f3 in the productions'of [a] followed by a nasal sound, the values obtained were respectively 3314.3 (samekoms), 3027.3 (kom) and 2825.0,(ongemaklik) hz. the first two values of [a] differ with 300 hz and the first and the latter with 500 hz. the values obtained for [a] in rok (2662.7 hz) and word (2472.0 hz) also vary considerably. with fl, 2 and 3, the median of the formant values of the subject was significantly higher than the median and formant values of the control group in production of [aj. discussion the study aimed to collect exploratory data on the speech of a subject with an undefined acquired neuromotor speech disorder. the initial findings indicate that the subject displays a unique combination of symptoms, different from both aos and dysarthria, namely inconsistent distortion of temporal and spatial dimensions, prolongations, variable rate and articulatory telescoping. these are all symptoms of neuromotor speech disorders which also occur in aos and dysarthria. however, they occurred in the absence of the core or salient features of these two disorders, namely slow effortfull speech with self-initiated attempts to correct errors, false starts and restarts, substitutions and distorted substitutions which is characteristic of aos according to most current authors and muscle tone disorders and involuntary movements which affects articulation, respiration, phonation, resonance and sometimes vegetative functions as well. despite the above characteristics, the subject was perceived as highly intelligible. there were no clear tendencies that could be identified in the speech of the subject on examination of the temporal features. the duration measurements (vowel and utterance) revealed longer than normal duration, however, the utterances were not perceived by the listeners as abnormally long. one can possibly attribute the longer durations to the nature of the material used. the nonsense syllables could facilitate careful productions. no conclusions can be drawn with regard to vot due to the rare occurrence of errors. the temporal features also showed no clear tendencies with regard to context sensitivity. this is contrary to the findings for aos, cerebellar dysarthria and lower motor neuron dysarthria which displayed diverse patterns of context sensitivity (van der merwe, 1986; brittz, 1994; van der merwe, et al., 1995). however, the lack of context sensitivity, might be characteristic of this particular disorder. this particular issue needs further research before any conclusions can be reached. the distorted quality in the subject's speech could not be explained by vot errors as they rarely occurred. the extended vd and ud might have contributed to the pertable 9: average formant frequencies (in hz) of the control data and the subject of [a] calculated average formant frequency (hz) formants control data subject formant 1 679.2 638.4 formant 2 1113.7( 1421.2 formant 3 2479.8 2668.2 table 10: average formant frequencies (in hz) of the control data and the subject of [ ] calculated average formant frequency (hz) formants control group subject formant 1 373.8 542.3 formant 2 805.6 1652.3 formant 3 2333.2 2860.2 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 96 dunay schmulian, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald ception of distortion in speech production. the formant analysis, however, revealed more about the subject's particular speech pattern. despite the high intelligibility, there was a combination of acoustic features in the vowels that were not characteristic of normally produced afrikaans vowels in a similar phonetic context. inconsistent distortions occurred due to inaccurate direction and range of movement. firstly, absolute formant values of fl, f2 and f3 varied considerably between productions. the first formant is most responsive to changes in mouth opening. speech sounds requiring small mouth openings have low frequency first formants. conversely, open mouth sounds are characterised by relatively high first frequency formants (borden, harris & raphael, 1994). while the subject's first formants are quite low in comparison to the controls, it can be proposed that the subject has a relatively small opening of the mouth for [a] and [o] which are back vowels and that possibly require relatively greater mouth opening. the second formant is most responsive to changes in the oral cavity. tongue backing or lip activity might lower the frequency of this formant, as these conditions occur in areas of high velocity, but any tongue or jaw movement which would narrow the region in the oral cavity, where the pressure is relatively high, would result in raising the frequency of the second formant (borden et al., 1994). as the subject's second formant values are higher than those of the control group, a possible explanation can be a too narrow region in the oral cavity due to excessive jaw and/or lip activity while producing [a] and [a]. the third formant is responsive to front versus back constriction of the articulators (borden et al., 1994), with the subject showing consequent difficulty in this area. this confirms the subject's difficulty with the relative mouth opening, oral cavity space and front versus back constriction of the articulators. this limited range of movement could contribute to the articulatory telescoping present in the subject's speech. vowels are perceptually differentiated according to the ratio of relationships between formant frequencies fl, f2 and f3 and not their absolute values (minifie, hixon & williams, 1973). the f1/f2 relationship is the most important for the perception of vowels (minifie et al., 1973). this does not imply that the interpersonal differences in formant relationships do not occur, but these differences should vary within the degrees of freedom or equivalence barriers (sharkey & folkins, 1985). with regard to formant relationships for [a], statistical analysis indicated deviant fl/2 and f2/3 relationships with only fl/3 falling within normal criteria. the typical coarticulatory patterns found in afrikaans, provide a possible explanation for the [a] sounding better than acoustic analysis indicated. with [a], fl/2 and fl/3 fall within normal intervals, even though the absolute formant frequencies showed deviation from the norm. the f2/3 deviated from the norm. it can therefore be assumed that the deviant formant relationships that did occur, led to the perception of distorted vowel quality and not . misperception of the vowel as being another vowel. but dominating the subject's symptom profile, and possibly underlying it, is the distortion of spatial and temporal dimensions in his speech. the distortion in vowel quality, the variable segmental duration and occasional errors in interarticulator synchronisation, could possibly be attributed to a disorder in motor programming of tone, rate, direction and range of movement (van der merwe, 1997). the finding that these symptoms occurred inconsistently might confirm this hypothesis. the unique combination of symptoms as found in this subject, might constitute one form of a pure motor programming speech disorder. the observation made by the listener panels that the subject sounded like a second language speaker, which he is not, is perhaps significant. in the literature, mention is made of a foreign accent syndrome which is an acquired neurogenic disorder (blumstein, alexander, ryalls, katz & dworetzky, 1987; takayama, sugishita, kido, ogawa, & akiguchi, 1993). it is possible that this case is an example of this syndrome. similar abnormalities in vowel production was identified in these cases. the basis of this syndrome, however, needs careful further research. the true nature of disorders in motor programming as postulated by the four level framework (van der merwe, 1997) needs to be clarified by future research. all possible cases should be studied with regard to site of lesion and symptomatology (acoustic and perceptual). the four level framework (van der merwe, 1997) should be considered as a basis for the comparison of the symptoms in these cases with that of other neuromotor disorders as a guide to a better understanding of the complex nature of acquired neurogenic communication disorders. the four level framework developed by van der merwe (1997), offers a novel view on neurogenic communication disorders, generating new questions to be answered by future research in the hope that "our gained insight will consequently assist in optimising clinical assessment and intervention" (van der merwe, 1997 : 19). references allen, g.i. & tsukahara, n. (1974). cerebrocerebellar communication systems. physiological reviews, 54, 957-997. blumstein, s.e., alexander, m.p., ryalls, j.h., katz, w. & dworetzky, b. (1987). on the nature of the foreign accent syndrome: a case study. brain and language, 31, 215-244. borden, g.j., harris, k.s. & raphael, l.j. (1994). speech science primer (3rd ed). baltimore: williams & wilkins. brittz, h.c. (1994). die invloed van kontekstuele faktore op sekere temporale parameters in disartriese spraak: 'n vergelyking tussen serebellere en onderste motor neuron disartrie. unpublished b. communication pathology research report. university of pretoria. ' brooks, v.b. (1986). the neural basis of motor control. new york: oxford university press. darley, f.l., aronson, a.e., & brown, j.r. (1975). motor speech disorders. philadelphia: w.b. saunders company. gay, t. (1979). coarticulation in some consonant-vowel and consonant cluster vowel syllables. in b. lindblom & s. ohman (eds). frontiers of speech communication research. london: academic press. ingrisano, d. & weismer, g. (1979). [s] duration: methodological influences and linguistic variables. phonetica, 36, 32-43. itoh, m. & sasanuma, s. (1984). articulatory movements in apraxia of speech. in j.c. rosenbek, m.r. mcneil & a.e. aronson (eds.). apraxia of speech: physiology, acoustics, linguistics and management. san diego: college-hill press. / kent, r.d. & rosenbek, j.c. (1983). acoustic patterns in apraxia of speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 26, 231249. kertesz, a. (1982). western aphasia battery. grune & stratton: new york. klopper, k. (1983). spraakverstaanbaarheid by afrikaanse disartriese sprekers: 'n voorgestelde toets. unpublished b.communithe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) exploratory study of an undefined acquired neuromotor speech disorder within the context ^ t h e four level framework for speech sensorimotor control 97 cation pathology research report. university, of pretoria, ointe, l.l. (1982). neurogenic disorders of speech. in g.h. shames & d.h. wigg (eds.). human communication disorders: an introduction. columbus: charles e. merril publishing company. lisker, l. & abrahamson, a.a. (1964). a cross-language study of voicing in initial stops: acoustic measurements. word, 20, 384422. marsden, c.d. (1984). which motor disorder in parkinson's disease indicates the true motor function of the basal ganglia? in: ciba foundation symposium 107. functions of the basal ganglia. london: pitman. mcneil, m.r. & kent, r.d. (1987). relative timing of sentence repetition in apraxia of speech and conduction aphasia. in: j.h. ryalls (ed.). phonetic approaches to speech production in aphasia and related disorders. boston: collage-hill, little brown and company. mcneil, m.r., robin, d.a. & schmidt, r.a. (1997). apraxia of speech: definition, differentiation, and treatment. in m.r. mcneil (ed) clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders. new york: thieme medical publishers. minifie, f.d., hixon, t.j. & williams, f. (1973). normal aspects of speech, hearing and language. new jersey: prentice-hall. prutting, c.a. & kirschner, d.m., in t.m. gallagher & c.a. prutting (1983). pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. college-hill press, inc.: san diego. sharkey, s.g. & folkins, j.w. (1985). variability of lip and jaw movements in children and adults: implications for the development of speech motor control. journal of speech and hearing research, 28, 8-15. takayama, y., sugishita, m., kido, t., ogawa, m. & akiguchi, i. (1993). a case of foreign accent syndrome without aphasia caused by a lesion of the left precentral gyrus. neurology, 43, 1361-2. van der merwe, a. (1985). disartrie-ondersoekvorm. universiteit van pretoria. van der merwe, a. (1986). die motoriese beplanning van spraak by verbale apraksie. unpublished d.phil dissertation. university of pretoria. van der merwe, α., groenewald, e., van aardt, d., tesner, h.e.c. & grimbeek, r.j. (1993). die formantpatrone van afrikaanse vokale soos geproduseer deur manlike sprekers. south african journal of linguistics, 11(2), 71-79. van der merwe, α., groenewald, e., brittz, h.c. & grimbeek, r.j. (1995). differentiating the levels of dysfunction in acquired neuromotor speech disorders: the context sensitivity of temporal features in apraxia of speech, cerebellar dysarthria and lower motor neuron dysarthria. proceedings of the british aphasiology society biennial international conference 12th 14th september 1995, p.16. van der merwe, a. (1997). a theoretical framework for the characterization of pathological speech sensorimotor control. in m.r. mcneil (ed.). clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders. new york: thieme medical publishers. yorkston, km., beukelman, d.r. & bell, k.r. (1988). clinical management of dysarthric speakers. texas: pro-ed. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) our real problem is in t h e hearts and minds of men. our real problem is in the hearts and minds of men. it is not a problem of physics but of ethics man's skills has outstripped his morals. his engineering has leaped ahead of his wisdom. we cannot cancel or call back his scientific advances, but we can, and we must, if the world is to survive, help man to catch up. in god's name, if you still believe in god, take him seriously, and somehow get control of what science has given the world, or else we shall likewise perish!" albert einstein. do you face advancing technology with apprehension and a little fear? this need not be! if you have a supplier who will help you all the way! for more than 20 years teknimed have brought the latest and most advanced solutions out of an enormous range of hearing health care products to dispensers, schools, ent specialists and hospitals all over south africa. we do everything to make your task as painless as possible by providing our very best product, service, price, training and after-sales service. from anything like a simple earlight, to custom acoustic enclosures, you can rely on us to give you an efficient and honest service! if you would like to find our more about what our team of specialists can do for you, write phone fax or email us at: teknimed enterprises cc ρ 0 box 662, somerset west, 7129 tel 0 2 1 8 5 2 1619. fax 0 2 1 8 5 1 3 8 3 3 . e-mail: teknimed@ilink.nis.za the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mailto:teknimed@ilink.nis.za pitch in esophageal speech l.w. l a n h a m , ph.d. (witwatersrand) head, department of phonetics & general linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg and w . a . kerr, m . r . c . s . , d . l . o . ( l o n d o n ) department of otorhinolaryngology, johannesburg hospital. (head: d.r. haynes) summary most reports on pitch in esophageal speech emphasize that it is low-pitched with a measured fundamental frequency rarely higher than 100 cps. our investigations show, however, that much esophageal 'phonation' lacks periodicity and,' therefore, a fundamental frequency (i.e. pitch in the accepted sense). an auditory impression of pitch modulation can, nevertheless, be created by physical properties other than a varying harmonic structure. our sample includes a rare case of truly high-pitched esophageal phonation with a fundamental frequency in the upper limit of the voice an octave higher than the highest reported in the literature. high-pitched phonation apparently requires a vibratory source in a 'mode' different from that of low-pitched phonation and should therefore be distinguished from it in discussing pitch in esophageal voice. opsomming meeste verslae oor esofageale spraak beklemtoon dat dit 'n lae toonhoogte het met 'n gemete grondtoon frekwens wat selde hoer is as 100 c.p.s. ons ondersoeke toon egter aan dat in baie gevalle die esofageale fonasie nie periodies varieer nie, en dat daar dus nie sprake kan wees van 'n grondtoon (d.i. toonhoogte in die aanvaarde sin) nie. die gehoorlike indruk van toonhoogte modulasie kan nietemin ook veroorsaak word deur fisiese eienskappe ander as harmoniese veranderende struktuur. die toonhoogte in een van die ondersoekte gevalle was uitsonderlik hoog, met 'n grondtoon in die boonste grensgebied van die stem, 'n oktaaf hoer as die hoogste tot dusver vermeld in literatuur. hoe toonhoogte fonasie vereis blykbaar 'n vibrerende bron in 'n vibrasietoestand wat verskil van die van 'n lae toonhoogte fonasie, en moet dus onderskei word van 'n lae toonhoogte in die verhandeling van toonhoogte in esofageale spraak. while the current literature provides a fair consensus as to the limitations of pitch in esophageal speech, certain anomalies and contradictions are nevertheless apparent. our evidence suggests that this arises from a failure to recognize that a measurable fundamental frequency (the physical correlate of pitch perception in normal voice) is often lacking, or only intermittently present, tydskrif van die suid-afrikaans vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, deseber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 l.w. lanham and w.a. kerr in the typical 'low-pitched' esophageal 'voice'. it is nevertheless necessary to account for the weak auditory impression of pitch variation in esophageal voice even without a measurable fundamental frequency. in distinguishing between pitchless 'phonation' and the esophageal voice with sustained, true pitch, we find in the data offered in the literature (in particular, kytta 7 ) and in our own sample, evidence of an upper limit to the pitch range in esophageal voice at least an octave higher than the highest frequency previously reported (see below). such high pitch is only achievable by a small number of laryngectomees who have both a high-pitch and low-pitch 'mode'. the suggestion that there are two apparently disjoint modes in esophageal phonation leads us ton investigate the possibility of two correspondingly different states of the cricopharyngeus muscle in vibration. methodology the kay sonagraph (model 6061 -b) was used to analyse the physical properties of esophageal voices in the manner described in kerr and lanham6. as indicated there, narrow-band spectrograms show harmonic structure in narrow lines on the horizontal axis (see, for example, spectrogram c2, narrow, 12-18 in this paper); wide-band spectrograms show each release burst in a sequence of vibratory cycles by vertical lines whose regularity and timing can be measured on the horizontal axis (± 12.33 cm = 1 second). fundamental frequency is measured as the interval between successive lower harmonics using frequency-scale magnification for greater accuracy (possible error is in the region of ± 5 hz). all of our spectrograms are cuts from the flow of speech in normal conversation or, in one case, from an attempt at singing. three of our cases are presented spectrographically here. one of them (case c) provided still radiographs drawn from extensive still and cineradiographic investigation of the site and mechanism of this case's two voices 'low' and 'highpitched' phonation. these will be found in kerr and lanham6, (p. 100.) case a (female, aged 61) underwent laryngectomy in 1953 when more than the usual segment of trachea was removed. she received speech therapy over a period of 6 months, but no great improvement was recorded. case β (female, aged 61) underwent laryngectomy in 1950; the infrahyoid muscles were preserved and overlapped to strengthen pharyngeal repair. she received speech therapy shortly after the operation and was greatly helped by it. case c (female, aged 62) underwent laryngectomy in 1951 with surgery similar to that of case b. she received speech therapy after the operation, but believes that it was largely due to her own efforts that she 'found her voice'. . / a comparison of findings in studies dealing with pitch in esophageal speech 1. fundamental frequencies and pitch ranges. reports on pitch in esophageal speech give measured fundamental frequencies and pitch ranges which are low in comparison with normal laryngeal speech (a full octave lower according to snidecor and curry1 0), but the extent of the pitch range may actually be greater than that of normal speakers a differjournul of the south african spccch ami hearing association, vol. 22, dcccmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pitch in esophageal speech 33 ence of 13.21 tones against 10.5 tones according to these authors. obvious anomalies do, however, arise in reports of this kind. after reporting the abnormally wide frequency range of superior esophageal speakers in their sample, curry and snidecor1 continue: '. . . the esophageal speakers were nonetheless considered to have a restricted pitch range . . .' and 'the frequency measured indicated a considerably greater movement than was apparent in pitch to the listener.' in citing a statement by van den berg: 'the speech of a clever patient sometimes gives the illusion of agreeable changes of pitch which objectively are not present' these authors do not fully explore the obvious contradiction. kytta7 states: 'the auditory observation group did not in fact notice any appreciable frequency variation . . . however, measurement of the fundamental frequency showed an unexpectedly large variation, 3-4 tones. ..'. anomaly and contradiction is, however, partly explained by the conclusions to which our studies have led us: (a) much 'low-pitched' esophageal phonation is actually pitchless, but does have measurable rates of vibration without periodicity (i.e. repeated cycles) and it is these aperiodic vibrations which are often measured, (b) the voices of many speakers have pitchless stretches interspersed with stretches in which periodicity is achieved and lower harmonics are discernible, (c) an auditory impression of pitch modulation can be achieved by varying prosodic properties other than a harmonic pattern, (d) the most effective pitch modulation is achieved by a small number of laryngectomees who can produce 'highpitched' phonation with pitch ranges considerably higher than those reported in the literature. in this article the main focus is on high-pitched phonation which has properties sufficiently distinct to warrant separation from the common lowpitched esophageal phonation. these properties apparently correspond to a different state or mode of the vibratory source. the major differences are found in pitch range and in a sustained harmonic structure with most of the energy concentrated in harmonics rather than in concomitant aperiodic components. spectrographic evidence of high-pitched phonation is found in the spectrograms below: c2 (narrow) and, less distinctly, ci (narrow). these demonstrate a clear harmonic structure up to 2 khz, a measurable fundamental varying between 215 and 317 and, therefore, pitch in the accepted sense of the term. (to our knowledge, the highest reported frequencies in esophageal speech are an estimate of 185 cps by damste3 and a measured 135.5 cps by curry and snidecor1.) our data on low-pitched esophageal voices show that the majority, including some highly intelligible ones, either totally lack a measurable fundamental frequency, or intermittently, even spasmodically, present short stretches of a syllable or less with a few lower harmonics and considerable aperiodic energy between discrete harmonic energies. a minority are capable of sustaining a discernible harmonic structure over several syllables and all these vibrations occur at rates which are within the 'pitch ranges' reported for esophageal speech, e.g.: 32 to 72 in kytta7, 17 to 135 in curry and snidecor1, 50 to 100 in van den berg and moolenaar-bijl13. the output of these slow rates of vibration of the pseudoglottis is, therefore, aperiodic more often than tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheekde, vol. 22, deseber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 l.w. lanham and w.a. kerr journl of the south african spch a hearing association, vol. 22, dcmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pitch in esophageal speech 35 ckb7. b1 (miiwi p oh make her a home (sung > * tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheekde, vol. 22, deseber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 l.w. lanham and w. a. kerr journl of the south african spch a hearing association, vol. 22, dcmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pitch in esophageal speech 37 periodic, but auditory impressions of pitch variation in esophageal speech are not necessarily dependent on periodicity; our evidence suggests that differ-, ences in intensity, duration, etc., can compensate in a limited way in giving an impression of pitch modulation (see our discussion below on case b's attempt at singing). plomp8 reports that in normal laryngeal speech, pitch is determined by the lower harmonics; fry5 states that it is the fundamental frequency which determines pitch, but the ear interprets the interval between lower harmonics as the pitch if there is no energy present in the first harmonic. as true pitch is a potential property of esophageal speech (particularly when it is 'high-pitched') we propose that esophageal phonation be recognized as pitchless where there is no evidence of lower harmonics and a fundamental frequency, or these are only spasmodically present over very short stretches. 2. characteristics of low-pitched esophageal speech in attempting to characterise low-pitched esophageal phonation we use kytta's data and our own. kytta's discussion reveals the basis for discrepancies between his interpretation of pitch properties and ours: in the spectrogram in fig. 3, p. 31, labelled 'spectrographs analysis of the fundamental frequency of the fundamental' we see a vibratory pattern producing releases which are bursts of noise irregular in time and amplitude, and varying in complexity. our spectrogram b1 (narrow) shows a very similar pattern which we recognize as common in low-pitched esophageal speech (all kytta's spectrograms on pp. 57-63 are of the same type). but fig. 3, and our spectrogram b1 (narrow) above, both show a vibratory pattern lacking periodicity and devoid of harmonic structure, and a measurable fundamental; they do, however, show measurable rates of vibration and it is apparently this which kytta measures in stating the limits of the pitch range of his subjects. vibratory rates in this mode can be considerably faster (varying around 92 at β1 1-2), but do not necessarily acquire periodicity, nor a higher pitch because of this. fast vibration in this mode (up to roughly 110 in our data) tends to lose release bursts in continuous turbulence (see b2, wide and narrow, 1-3 and kytta's hiipi 0-0.25 on p. 63). in some, possibly a minority of esophageal speakers phonating in this mode, discrete lower harmonics (usually below 1 khz) do indeed emerge intermittently over short stretches often shorter than a syllable, but a good deal of energy in random aperiodic components partly obscures them. (in our data these intermittent harmonics occur in the range of 60 to 130 cps.) in respect of the quantity of periodicity in low-pitched esophageal phonation, yielding a measurable first harmonic, we find it difficult to accept curry and snidecor's2 finding that 59.5% out of 61.4% total phonation is periodic. 3· characteristics of high-pitched esophageal phonation. high-pitched esophageal phonation is characterized by sustained harmonic structure over more than one syllable; high concentration of energy in the lower harmonics as distinct from accompanying noise; and the pitch range, the lowest limit of which is probably in the region of 150 cps and the highest tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheekde, vol. 22, deseber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 l.w. lanham and w.a. kerr close to 400 cps (the latter frequency is seen in spectrogram dvi in kerr and lanham6, p. 92). in addition to our case c, kytta provides spectrographs evidence of high-pitched esophageal phonation similar to case c (but lacking the vibrato in the voice of the latter) on p. 50 (the utterance vaara) and p. 54 (tlodco). fundamental frequency in the former is roughly 170 cps at 0.25 and 162 cps at the peak of the first syllable in the latter. kytta, however, fails to interpret his data correctly and gives the highest recorded fundamental frequency in his sample as 72 cps. case c has a pitch range between 210 cps and nearly 400 cps; the fundamental frequency in spectrogram c2 is 270 cps at 1.3 and 317 cps at 16.4. (notice, however, the break into low-pitched phonation at c2 3-8). another possible feature of high-pitched phonation is the significantly longer 'duration of air charge'. snidecor and curry 1 0 , reporting on mean duration of air charge in their sample, give a maximum of 2.25 sees. case c can repeat 'all my arms are sore'1 for approximately 4.5 sees. in tape-recorded conversation her longest stretch without obviously recharging her esophagus is 3.7 sees. an analysis of the three voices in briefly reviewing our three cases presented here we note that radiography locates the vibratory source at the cricopharyngeus for all. (the postulation of an upper pharyngeal vibrator for case c's high-pitched phonation made in kerr and lanham6, has been refuted by recent cineradiography.) the voices of cases a and β are confined to low-pitched phonation with no evidence of discernible harmonics in the voice of the former and virtually none in the latter. case a's voice is croaky, low in power and intelligibility and gives no auditory impression of pitch modulation. there is believed to be some loss of muscular function in the inferior pharyngeal constrictor. spectrogram a1 shows very slow, highly irregular rates of vibration; at a1 1-3 there are 9 vibrations over some 17 csecs at rates ranging between 30 and 61. case β on the other hand has a highly efficient voice of considerable power and high intelligibility although unpleasantly harsh. vibrations are faster and more regular; a variation of ± 11 around a mean rate of 92 at β1 1-2 and ±2 around a mean rate of 45 at b1 11-12. case b's voice is capable of weak impressions of pitch modulation and the sung and spoken versions of the same utterance are shown in spectrograms b1 and b2 respectively as evidence of the physical properties of 'pitch' in aperiodic low-pitched esophageal speech. the sung version differs in the ' following respects: (a) where the highest pitch is attempted (make at b2 5-8) the syllable duration is more than doubled, (b) the rate of vibration increases by nearly 40% at b2 5-8 and approximately 30% at b2 11 (the vowel nucleus of her), (c) the intensity of the prominent syllables is greater by 3.3 db for the vowel nucleus of make and 1.3 for the nucleus of home, (d) more energy 1 this utterance was chosen because it lacks stop (plosive) consonants. the articulation of these consonants has been shown by van den berg, et a l . 1 2 to present opportunities for filling the esophagus. 1 journl of the south african spch a hearing association, vol. 22, dcmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pitch in esophageal speech 39 is located in higher frequencies (above 3 khz); an impression of this difference can be gained at b2 1-3. apart from (b), which does not create discernible harmonics to any extent, the other differences, probably collectively, contribute to the auditory impression of higher pitch. case c has two 'voices' (low and high-pitched esophageal phonation) and spectrograms c2 are produced in order to show how they alternate in the continuum of speech and reveal the basis of the strong impression of pitch modulation which, over the telephone, can deceive even the most experienced ear as to the true nature of the voice. as seen at c2 3-8, where the second, high-pitched voice breaks and the first voice takes over, case c's first voice gives no evidence of a harmonic structure. the first voice is heard as lowpitched and it is the alternation between the two voices which contributes to the strong impression of wide pitch variation. case c reports that her second voice is the product of strenuous, sustained effort in the months after laryngectomy and obviously requires a much higher degree of neuromuscular control; the vibrato effect best seen in ci (narrow), is evidence of this. with declining morale in recent years case c tends to lapse into her first voice for longer and longer stretches. the general impression of case c's voice is that it is a high-pitched woman's voice (spectrographically at c2 13-17 it is strikingly similar to female laryngeal phonation), but extreme, often abrupt, variation in power is a feature. pitch change is often sharp, sudden and somewhat unpredictable. the abrupt changes usually correspond to a change from one voice to another; spectrogram c2 shows this with c2 at 1 and 12-21 being the second voice with a onesyllable break into the first voice at 3-8. state and function of the esophageal sphincter in vibration the site and mechanism of the two modes of phonation in case c's voice may be examined by reference to radiographs on p. 100 in ken and lanham6. in that article they are numbered d1 d4. d2 and d3 represent phonation in the first (low-pitched) and second (high-pitched) modes respectively, d4 outlines the cricopharyngeus immediately after phonation has ceased. in d3 the shadow over the lower three-fourths of a club-shaped head of the cricopharyngeus outlines a channel of air which rises and tapers abruptly to a point of occlusion in the upper quarter of the clubhead. the constriction ring within the esophageal sphincter is apparently located here. in d2 the constriction ring is less easily identifiable, but an air channel is seen above the protruding fold (marked a) and forward of the lower third of the inferior anterior edge of the clubhead. blurring at the upper half of this anterior edge suggests a comparatively large mass of the inner margin of the sphincter involved in vibration, which is not the case in d3. this latter state is consistent with highfrequency vibration in which only a short stretch of a tightly constricted inner margin is involved. in an attempt at simulating the second mode of vibration in the form of a labial trill, l.w.l. produced, in the manner described below, a spectrogram tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheekde, vol. 22, deseber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 l.w. lanham and w.a. kerr fairly similar to ci (narrow) 6-10, which showed a fundamental around a mean of 400 cps with undulating harmonics. it was necessary to make a very tight labial closure with strong intra-oral pressure bursting through a very short stretch (about .5 cm) which produced a high-frequency labial trill. essential requirements are that each lip present a stiff, tense, edge which is relatively thin. with more flaccid, thicker edges labial trilling becomes slower and less regular, a longer stretch is involved in vibration and periodicity is easily lost. for high-frequency trilling the level of effort required to achieve the balance between air pressure, muscle tension and the configuration of the vibrating edge appears to match that required for case c's second voice and is as difficult to maintain at an even level. in comparison with radiograph d4 (kerr and lanham6) in which there is no vibration, phonation in d2 and d3 is seen to involve a shortening of the 'neck' behind the clubhead of the cricopharyngeus and a lifting and flattening of the head (contrast the drooping head in d4). the head is flattened in the vertical plane more in d3 than in d2 suggesting tighter constriction. taking account of the differences shown in the radiographs referred to above, we suggest that first-mode and second-mode vibrations differ in mechanism in the following ways: in the former a relatively thick, fairly relaxed inner margin of the sphincter is drawn into closure or close approximation in a muscular movement not much different from that required for esophageal burping in a normal speaker; consequent vibrations involve a comparatively large mass of the cricopharyngeus. second-mode vibration, however, requires a much more highly controlled movement which produces greater tension along the inner margin and presents a stiffer, firmer edge at the point where vibration takes place. much less of the margin is involved in high-frequency, low amplitude vibration with only small quantities of esophageal air released in the process. this could account for the significantly longer 'duration of air charge' in this type of phonation. the distended upper end of the esophagus suggests high intra-esophageal pressure. conclusion in the discussion above it has not been our intention to present a case for two mutually exclusive categories of esophageal phonation, but to highlight a type of esophageal phonation of which only a limited number of laryngectomees is capable. this 'second mode' of pseudoglottal vibration is the product of a relatively intact musculature and considerable effort both in acquiring and maintaining it. we.suggest, however, that in one or more of the parameters of esophageal phonation the two modes are discontinuous states and not merely different points on the same scales. there is, for example, no evidence that over any voiced' stretch in esophageal speech a gradational move from one to the other mode is possible (by progressively increasing muscle tension along the margin of the esophageal sphincter). spectrogram c2 (wide) shows how case c moves from one mode to another by a discrete abrupt change; in fact, 2 v a n den berg's 'high pitch' is not equatable to our second mode in esophageal phonation. his upper limit of the fundamental frequency is 85 cps and is said to coincide with high intensity and stronger air flow. journl of the south african spch a hearing association, vol. 22, dcmber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pitch in esophageal speech 41 we have no evidence at all of harmonic energies in case c's first, low-pitched voice and, therefore, no evidence of an ability to shift gradually from low to high pitch. other investigators have suggested that one 'setting' of the pseudoglottis in phonation cannot be phased gradually into another. van den berg et a l . 1 2 , state: '. . . he always separated the section with a high pitch2 from that with a low pitch by a new breath and a new injection of a i r . . .'. there is evidence, therefore, that for stretches of, possibly, the duration of an air charge, the esophageal speaker is locked into a particular mode of phonation and, if a harmonic structure is present, into maintaining more or less the same pitch. references 1. curry, e.t. and snidecor, j.c. (1961): physical measurement and pitch perception in esophageal speech. the laryngoscope. lxxi: 415-424. 2. curry, e.t. and snidecor, j.c. (1973): fundamental frequency characteristics of japanese asai speakers. the laryngoscope. lxx1ii: 17591763. 3. damste, p.h. (1958): oesophageal speech after laryngectomy. thesis, print. gebr. hoitsema. groningen. 4. damste, p.h. (1959): the glotto-pharyngeal press. speech pathology and therapy. 2:70-76. 5. fry, d.b. (1970): prosodic phenomena. in malmberg, b. ed., manual of phonetics. amsterdam. 6. kerr, w.a. and lanham, l.w. (1973): anatomical and spectrographic analysis of the voice in disease: a report on five cases. j. sa speech & hear, association. 20:81-107. 7. kytta, j. (1964): finnish oesophageal speech after laryngectomy. acta oto-laryngologica, supplementum 195.vammala. stockholm. 8. plomp, r. (1966): experiments on tone perception. van gorcum and comp. netherlands. 9. robe, e.y., moore, g.p., andrews, a.h., holinger, p.h. (1956): a study of the role of certain factors in the development of speech after laryngectomy. the laryngoscope. lxvi: 382-401. 10. snidecor, j.c. and curry, e.t. (1959): temporal and pitch aspects of superior esophageal speech. the annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology. lxviii: 623-636. 11. tato, j.m. (1954): study of the sonospectrographic characteristics of the voice in laryngectomized patients. acta oto-laryngologica. 44: fasc. 5-6. 12. van den berg, j.w., moolenaar-bijl, a.j., damste, p.h. (1958): oesophageal speech. folia phoniatrica. 10:65-81. 13. van den berg, j.w. and moolenaar-bijl, a.j. (1959): cricopharyngeal sphincter, pitch, intensity and fluency in oesophageal speech. prac. oto-rhino-laryngol. 21:4. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheeikimde, vol. 22, deseniber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) modern hearing aids (pty.) limited 305 rand central 305 22-4855 165 jeppe street/straat 165 johannesburg 2001 box/bus 83, johannesburg 2000 the latest from telex the tw2/tdr2 fm auditory training system: featuring: teachers microphone and transmitter all in the size of a hearing aid. student direct receiver for the following modes of operation: — 1) fm and hearing aid with a control for adusting fm relative to hearing aid sound pick-up. 2) telephone or loop pick-up alone. 3) hearing aid alone. peak output, compression and tone controls are also provided. uses a dry battery or rechargeable cell. automatic built in squelch provided. operating frequency changed by plug in module allowing free movement from one classroom to another. no extra wires required — no cross talk between classrooms — 32 channels available. our services also include: 1) the supplying of the latest directional discrimina ting hearing aids for mid-medium-severe hearing losses. 2) making the most comfortable instant earmoulds for our patients — while they wait. 3) first service facilities covered by a 3 month guarantee. other products include: 1) screening and diagnostic audiometers. 2) sound measuring instruments. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. , december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) relationship between v e r b a l l a n g u a g e and symbolic play — a case study denise segal, m . a . ( s p e e c h pathology) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) department speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary the linguistic ability and play of a language-impaired child were analysed to determine whether a breakdown in symbolic play occurs together with a language deficit. observation of play was conducted at the child's nursery school (unstructured situation) and in a situation designed to elicit specific play behaviours (structured situation). imaginative play and its concomitants — affect, mood variability, concentration, aggression and interaction — were rated along descriptive scales, while each individual play unit was scored for organization of behaviour. syntactic, semantic and phonological aspects of language were recorded during free play and analyzed within a syntactic framework. the normal developmental sequence provided the baseline of comparison for both language and play. results indicated a developmental lag in play and a linguistic deviation from the normal pattern, which supported the possibility of a general representational deficit. a method for incorporating symbolic play into a language programme was suggested and the necessity for normative studies in this area was stressed. opsomming die linguistiese vermoe en spel van 'n taalgestremde kind is ontleed om vas te stel of 'n versteuring in simboliese spel gelyk met 'n taaltekort plaasvind, al dan nie. waarneming van spel het by die kind se kleuterskool plaasgevind (ongestruktuurde situasie) en ook in 'n situasie beraam om spesifieke speelgedrag te ontlok (gestruktueerde situasie) verbeeldingsryke spel en die konkomitante verskynsels — gevoelsinhoud, wisselvalligheid van stemming, konsentrasie, aggressie, en aksie en reaksie — is volgens beskrywende maatstawe beordeel, terwyl elke adsonderlike speleenheid vir organisasie van gedrag opgeteken is. sintaktiese, semantiese en fonologiese aspekte van taal is gedurende vry spel aangeteken en binne 'n sintaktiese raamwerk geanaliseer. die normale ontwikkelingsvolgorde is gebruik as die basis van vergelyking vir sowel taal as spel. 'n ontwikkelingsvertraging in spel en 'n linguistiese afwyking van die normale is deur die resultate aangedui, wat die moontlikheid ondersteun van 'n algemene weergewende tekort. 'n metode om simboliese spel in 'n taalprogram te integreer is voorgestel en die noodsaaklikheid vir normatiewe studies in die gebied beklemtoon. it is generally agreed upon by researchers that language and cognition are correlated although the nature of the relationship is controversial. recent investigations2· 3 ' 4 ' 2 1 · 3 1 ' 3 2 have tended to support piaget's hypothesis2 4 that language is based in prior cognitive growth. this suggests that a linguistic breakdown may be a reflection of an underlying cognitive disturbance thereby providing a conceptual framework within which to view diagnosis of, and therapy for, the language-impaired child.1' 2 1 definitions and classifications of play are numerous and variable according to theoretical orientation but the viewpoint adopted in the present investigation is that of piaget. 2 5 he theorized 2 4 that children die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 denise segal aged two to four years develop a general representational ability which manifests in all symbolic systems, namely, deferred imitation, imagery, drawing, dreaming, symbolic play and language. he defined symbolic play as ". . . the deformation and subordination of reality to the desires of the self".2 5 l u n z e r 1 7 considered it to be the natural medium of behaviour and of intellectual growth at that period which coincides with speech development. although the cause-effect relationship is undetermined, 1 2 , 1 5 ' 1 7 language and symbolic play are believed to share a common cognitive base and to function similarly as 'signifiers' to an absent content. while language enables man . . . ίο represent objects and situations in their absence . . . (p. 207) 1 7 symbolic play allows the child to use one object, action or event, to represent another object, action or e v e n t . 1 0 ' 1 9 ' 2 8 it is thought that language at first accompanies play until the child's naming of an object emancipates word from object. 7 with increased play complexity and the substitution of play names for real names at about three years of age, the child is able to plan an activity verbally.7' 1 8 since the actions involved in play resemble the significant content, they are believed to be more easily acquired than wordmeanings which are arbitrary.1' 2 4 this suggests that in some children a linguistic deficit may also present with a breakdown in symbolic play. piaget (p. 338) 2 5 described play as . . . an exercise of action schemes . . . in which assimilation predominates over accommodation. thus the child utilizes and incorporates an aspect of the environment into himself rather than imitating and adapting his own body to the social milieu. in 1951 piaget drew up a classification scheme comprising practice play, symbolic play and games with rules, each class characteristic of a particular developmental stage — sensori-motor, representational and concrete operational respectively. during the sensori-motor stages ii-v (from 2 to 28 months), the child's actions are exercised for pleasure alone. the genesis of the symbolic function occurs at about 18 months of age (sub-stage iv), with a subsequent developmental progression both in the organization of symbolic play behaviours, and in the degree of representational ability that they reflect.17· 2 4 · 3 1 piaget's scales, although theoretical and not intended for clinical usage, have been adapted by researchers such as l u n z e r 1 7 and bass et al1 for use in both clinical and natural settings. bass et al stressed that in truly symbolic behaviour, the child is aware of the distortion of reality whereby the action occurs out of context. initially, the action rather than the object is the symbol anil can be performed with objects inadequate to fulfil the goal (e.g. using a stick in a representation of eating), or in the absence of any objects (e.g. simulating eating with no external aids). at this stage, the child is able to perform these actions only upon himself, and is not able to attribute them to others until the representational stage has been reached. the representational stage comprises three substages, each including the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 39 different types of behaviour, according to their symbolic structure. during substage i, the child is able to project his own actions (type ia), and then his imitated action patterns (type ib) onto new objects. substage ii, involves representation by simply identifying one object with another, eg. picking up a stick and saying, "this can be a broom" ( t v p e 2a) and subsequently, by using bodily actions to assume another identity (type 2b). the child's ability to utilize individual objects symbolically within an integrated framework (type 3a) is apparent from three to four years of age. the value of symbolism in play subsequently diminishes in that it becomes differentiated into games with rules. thus while these substages and classes constitute an evolutionary continuum with no clear delineations1 5' they do allow for the study of the child's cognition through the observable mode of symbolic play. this is felt to be particularly appropriate for the language-impaired child where cognitive investigation through the linguistic medium would inevitably be unrepresentative of his true performance. . comparatively few studies have investigated the relationship between language and symbolic play, but the symbolic function has been shown to be impaired in linguistically deviant children.1' 1 5 ' 2 1 ' in addition, luria and yudovich, 1 8 and vygotskaia7 found progress in symbolic play as a result of speech improvement. however, further research is necessitated. the investigation by lovell, hoyle and siddall considered only duration of play behaviour which lunzer found to be an unreliable measure, whereas bass et al1 utilized a structured play situation only, thereby ignoring crucial aspects which have been found to be related to play. these comprise affect during play, concentration and aggression, as well as interaction and co-operation with both peers and adults. . it is controversial which aspects of play actually involve symbolic or abstract thought. el'konin 7 considered only dramatic play, the substitution of an imaginary situation to satisfy the child's needs. other investigators17' 3 0 have added constructive play whereby objects are manipulated in order to create. it was this latter viewpoint that was adopted by the present investigator. play like language, is learnt in relation to ongoing meaningful activities.13 the writer thus set out to study play . . . in the overall context of the individual and social actions of the child, (p. 339) the child's continuous interaction with his environment as an essential aspect of play has been emphasized since his spontaneous . . . direct collision . . . with reality (p. 228)7 enables him to assimilate reality to his limited cognitive schema. in accordance with singer, it is believed that certain defensive or conflict-reducing behaviours may also participate in this process. interaction with, and observation of adults, has been similarly emphasized in the development of play. this is believed to facilitate separation of an activity from an object, its generalization to new objects, and progressive abstraction of usage. die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 denise segal f r e y b e r g 8 also argued that the adult model serves as the catalyst for the development of latent skills which are basically within the child's capabilities, despite lacks in experience or cognition. whereas the parent initially directs the child's behaviour by verbal instruction, this is later internalized by the child. on the basis of foregoing findings, the aim of the present study was to analyse and to describe qualitatively the symbolic play and linguistic ability of a language-impaired child. more specifically, this study was designed to determine whether a linguistic breakdown would manifest with a corresponding breakdown in symbolic play. m e t h o d o l o g y aims 1. to describe qualitatively and to analyze the symbolic play and linguistic ability of a language-impaired child. 2. on the basis of the above, to determine whether a symbolic breakdown will co-occur with a language impairment. 3. if (2) above is supported, to determine whether the nature of the breakdown, that is, delay versus impairment, at the level of play corresponds with that at the language level (namely, impairment). 4. to determine whether play performance of this particular child differs in a structured versus an unstructured situation. 5. on the basis of the above findings, to devise a therapy programme for this child incorporating symbolic play within a linguistic framework. subject one male child (s) aged 3 years 8 months was selected as subject. he was the youngest of three children and had been diagnosed by a qualified speech therapist from the speech and hearing clinic at the university of the witwatersrand, johannesubrg, as presenting with an expressive language-impairment. s fulfilled the following criteria: 1. chronological age — s's chronological age fell within the range of that reported in normal development for a fully established linguistic system (3v2-4 years) and heightened symbolism in play (2-4 years). 1 7 · 2 4 2. mental age — due to s's inattention and lack of co-operation, a traditional iq measure could not be established however, s was assessed by his speech therapist to be of average intelligence according to his performance on non-verbal tasks. 3. s had no physical or primary emotional disability. 4. s had hearing within normal limits. 5. at the commencement of observation, s had been attending a nursery school (mornings only) for five months, thereby ensuring that he had adjusted to, and had become familiar with, the physical and social environment. 1 6. previous speech therapy could not be controlled for. for a year the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 41 prior to this study, s had attended a play group for languageimpaired children three times a week, while simultaneously receiving individual speech therapy twice a week at the speech and hearing clinic of the university of the witwatersrand. however, while the added stimulation with regard to language and play as well as experience with play materials would influence the child's performance, it would work against the proposed hypothesis of a deficit in play occurring together with a language deficit. general procedure 1. language: (a) syntax and semantics — a sample of the child's spontaneous oral language was elicited during his regular speech therapy period. play materials included toys and action pictures which have been found to facilitate speech in three and four-year olds. the entire session was tape recorded using a reel-to-reel tape recorder. s's regular therapist conducted the session with the experimenter (e) observing behind a one-way mirror. the therapist and ε together transcribed the utterances. (b) phonology — this was included only to ensure differentiation between a syntactic error and one which was phonologically based, for example, omission of copula versus omission of phoneme [s]. the phonetic inventory test was administered by both the therapist and e. 2. play: (a) unstructured play situation — ε visited the nursery school to familiarize herself with the environment and routine. play materials included all the indoor and out-door equipment present. children were not encouraged into conversation with her, and any questions were answered 'tersely but pleasantly'. this allowed her to become part of the situation as an 'object' offering nothing in return for a child's advances as would an assistant. this ensured that ε would exert minimal influence on the child's activity.1 5 ε observed the child during free play for twenty ten-minute periods which were randomly scattered over ten days. the number of ratings was sufficient to eliminate such variables as the individual's feelings at the time and fluctuations in performance,1 7 while brief periods minimized the influence of fatigue. an interval of at least five minutes was allowed between each observation period. ε transcribed the child's overt expressions, play behaviours, interactions and verbalizations. a random sample of one in every five of the play sessions was simultaneously observed but independently transcribed and rated by a second, similarly experienced observer, (b) structured play situation — this was carried out during one of the child's speech therapy periods after completion of observadie suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 denise segal tions in the unstructured situation. this prevented experience with play materials and examples of their symbolic usage from influencing the child's performance in the unstructured situation. the entire session which lasted 20 minutes was recorded on a reel-to-reel videotape. instructions were conveyed with the aid of linguistic and gestural cues and by means of example, in order to minimize the possible influence of receptive difficulties. rating scales a n d analysis of data: 1. language: (a) syntax and semantics — the child's language was analyzed in relation to the context and his ongoing activities. while a semantic analysis would appear to be more applicable in relation to cognitive assessment, a syntactic analysis was preferred on the basis of the view of crystal et al 6 that no semantic theory has been worked out sufficiently for descriptive studies of any general validity to have taken place. a standardized syntactic analysis allowed for the establishment of a quantitative language level as well as affording qualitative analysis. this was necessary in order to confirm the diagnosis of language-impairment and to ascertain whether delay occurred together with impairment. it is felt that if the interaction between language and symbolic play was being considered at a 'microscopic' (rather than a 'macroscopic') level, a semantic and pragmatic analysis would be more appropriate. this would involve analysing the functional language that occurs together with a particular type of play activity, for example, language that serves to extend the play activity as opposed to language that describes the ongoing activity. contrarily, the present study aimed to determine a language level, a play level, and only subsequently to establish whether similarities exist between these two behaviours in this particular child, this constitutes a 'macroscopic' or 'surface level' analysis. thus, semantics and pragmatics were included only to differentiate a semantically-based from a syntacticallybased error, as well as to exemplify unintelligible utterances by considering their intent within the play context. analysis included development sentence scoring (dss) and development sentence types (dst) comprising ^^developmental sentence analysis (dsa) devised by l e e . 1 3 from the corpus, the final fifty intelligible and consecutive complete sentences (with noun and verb in subject-predicate relationship) were selected for dss analysis. repeated, stereotyped and imitated utterances were excluded. it was agreed by the therapist (who was 'blind' to the aims of the study) and ε that s had acclimitized to the situation, and that his optimal performance was thereby included in the sample. the 68 pre-sentence the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 43 formulations from the corpus were included in a dst analysis. in addition, all s's utterances in which the target was a complete sentence (179) were analysed qualitatively in terms of linguistic trends as well as use and diversity of base structure and transformational rules. (b) phonology — the phonetic inventory test was analysed by ε together with a member of the phonetics and linguistics department at the university of the witwatersrand. 2. play: (a) unstructured play situation — play behaviour was rated along the dimension of imaginativeness, affect, mood variability, concentration, aggression and interaction with peers and adults, devised by singer. 3 3 in accordance with lunzer's 1 7 view that constructive play is a pretence or abstraction from reality, this type of play was included in the rating for imaginativeness. this dimension and those for affect and concentration each received one overall score for the most frequent behaviour within each ten-minute period. a score was assigned for each and every interactive and co-operative behaviour displayed since it was often impossible to assign only one descriptive term which would do justice to the diversity of these behaviours within any ten-minute period. the scale for aggression measured direct overt aggression as opposed to that manifest in make-believe play. here, intensity rather than frequency of behaviour was considered. it was felt that an intense aggressive attack against a peer, even if momentary, could hinder future interactions and therefore, social play. the rating from 1 4 on the aggression scale was altered to 1 5 to comply with scoring on the other scales. a low score of 1 was positive, indicating no aggression, while a high score of 5 was negative, describing a great deal of aggression. for all other scales the reverse occurred in that a score of 1 was the negative side of the scale, for example, indicating low imaginativeness in play. in addition to the above, each separate play unit was rated on a 9-point index for the organization of play behaviours devised by l u n z e r . 1 7 this comprises two subscales — adaptiveness in the use of play materials which measures the child's treatment of the play materials, and integration of behaviour which concerns the complexity of the play behaviour itself. this scale yielded validity and reliability scores of 0,73 and 0,75 respectively. significant iriterscorer correlations of 0,92; 0,97; 0,85 and 0,90 (p 0,05) were obtained by means of the pearson productmoment correlation coefficient, (b) structured play situation — play was rated on a modification of lunzer's scale devised, in an unpublished masters dissertation, by bass, brown and redmond. 1 this measured symbolic play die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 denise segal behaviours at the substages, and of the type described by p i a g e t . 2 4 results the dearth of normative studies yielding quantitative data necessitated a largely qualitative analysis. for both language and play the normal developmental sequence served as the baseline for comparison. chronology, as well as the child's present developmental stage allowed any delay in, or deviation from the norm to be noted. 1. language: s obtained a dss score of 5,44 which falls below the tenth percentile for his chronological a g e . 1 3 ' 1 4 his language usage was comparable to that of a child aged 3 years 1 month, indicating a delay of seven months. dst analysis demonstrated that s used the earlier acquired verb phrase (vp) more frequently than the noun phrase (np). although s did combine noun and modifier to constitute two-word np's, his vp constructions were more advanced, being three, four or five words in length. this indicated that s was in a transition stage between pre-sentence formulation and consistent sentence stage. qualitative analysis revealed a delay in language development as well as a deviation from the normal sequence (impairment). the base structure rule (s—>np+vp) was not fully established, diversity of structural type and frequency of usage were limited, while application of transformational rules was not consistently correct. 2.1 play behaviour: (a) imaginative play as rated on the scale devised by singer.33 from figure 1 it is evident that s's most frequently occurring play behaviour was either extremely unimaginative and stimulus-bound (a score of 1) or it included a few pretend elements with little originality or organization (a score of 2). (b) organization of play behaviours as rated on the scale devised by lunzer.17 figure 2 reveals that s most frequently used play materials '. . . in a manner recalling the play of infants' (57% of the time). cognizance was not taken of their physical or representational properties, and the integration of play behaviours was minimal with little achieved (a composite score of 2). s's play behaviour fell predominantly (89,9% of the time) within scores 2 6 which has been equated with·-'piaget's 'practice play'. he obtained a score of 7, comparable to the rudimentary stages of piaget's 'symbolic play' (scores 7-10) only 10,1% of the time. thus, on the basis of his performance, it appears as though s is functioning slightly below the range for his age level although still conforming with the normal developmental pattern. the peak of symbolic play at two-to-four years of a g e 1 7 ' 2 4 was not apparent from his performance in the unstructured situation. thesouth african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 45 20 18 16 14 12 key: 1 = extremely unimaginative. 2 = slightly imaginative. 3 = moderate amount of pretending. 4 = substantial pretend elements. 5 = high originality. 2 • ' • ' • l 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 imaginative play figure 1. frequency of occurrence of imaginative elements in play. (c) performance in structured play situation — this was included to determine whether s would be able to perform these activities when presented with appropriate stimuli for their elicitation. a qualitative analysis of s's performance demonstrated his ability to carry out activities equivalent to all stages except stage iiib. at all stages, the highest score attained (i 3 ), described integration of a number of separate behaviours within the framework of a fairly complex task. for example, in representational substage i, type ia, he put his hand into the box and lifted 'food' from the 'bowl' to the doll's mouth. the highest score achieved for adaptiveness in the use of play materials was a3, that is, the material was used with regard to its properties but in an obvious way. for example, in representational substage ii, type 2a, he built a tower and a train by placing blocks along a vertical and horizontal plane respectively. in one example only, he received higher scores of a5 for highly die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 denise segal 7 9 a 50 45 40 key: 2 = infant play; no integration. 3 = some regard for properties; no integration. 4 = some regard for properties; routine integration. 5 = obvious regard for properties; routine integration. 6 = obvious regard for properties; integrated framework. 7 = use transcends properties; integrated framework. 8 = use transcends properties; central theme. 9 = insightful use; central theme. 10 = insightful use; coherent sequence. 35 30 25 20 15 1 0 5 ' ' ' ' ' ' * j ι ο 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 organization of play behaviour figure 2. frequency ratings of categories of play organization. insightful usage of material and i 4 for the elaboration of a single theme, when he displayed types 2a and 2b concurrently (not included in scale by bass et al). here he represented an 'alarm clock' by combining two objects, a bubble-blower and a hair roller, while simultaneously producing a ringing sound. thus, s's performance at all stages, though inconsistent, was more advanced than that displayed in the nursery school. 2.2 associated play concomitants: (refer fig. 3) (a) affect — from figure 3a it can be seen that s scored 2 predominantly for affect, descriptive of only mild pleasure and interest accompanying 'desultory manipulation' of play mathe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 2 0 a 1 2 1 0 >< ο ζ ω d σ ω f* ο α. 20α 1 2 1 0 κευ: 1 = no interest or pleasure in play. 2 = mild pleasure and interest. 3 = moderate pleasure and interest. 4 = pleasure frequently expressed. 5 = extreme delight in play. 2 3 affect key: 1 = little attention to activities. 2 = superficial play — frequent change of activities. 3 = moderate interest activities changed once. 4 = good absorption in play — no change in activity. 5 = intense absorption in play. 0 1 2 3 4 b. concentration 2 0 a 1 2 1 0 6 key: 1 non-aggressive activity. 2 = play with aggressive toy. 3 = aggression directed at inanimate object. 4 = aggression directed at another child's toy. 5 = aggression directed at another child. aggression figure 3. frequency ratings of categories of affect, concentration and aggression. die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 denise segal terials. he did enjoy himself in the play situation (scores 3, 4 and 5) although this occurred infrequently. a pearson productmoment correlation coefficient demonstrated a significant relationship (r = 0,48; ρ 0,05) between play and affect. it has been argued that the child should display positive affect if the play in which he is engaged is inherently rewarding.8' 3 3 (b) concentration: — from figure 3b it is evident that s engaged predominantly (50% of the time) in 'superficial play', altering toys and activities frequently while looking around the room, staring passively, talking to the teacher, or wandering aimlessly. a further 35% of the time he displayed 'hyperactivity with no real interaction with play material'. a significant correlation (r = 0,46; ρ 0,05) was found between imaginative play and concentration. the child's generation of complex, interesting games, should create sufficient satisfaction for prolonged involvement. (c) aggression — it is apparent from figure 3c that s engaged predominantly in non-aggressive activities (a score of 1 occurred 45% of the time). however, an additional 45% was involved with directing a threat or physical attack against another child (scores 4 and 5 combined). no significant correlation was found between play and aggression. figure 4 demonstrates that s sought adult company more often than that of his peers. interaction with his peers varied from an apparent lack of trust to interact, resulting in avoidance of contact (a score of 1) to actively seeking their company (a score of 4). 20 1 4 1 2 1062il l 1 2 3 4 interaction j l — = interaction with peers = interaction with adults key: interaction with peers 1 = avoids contact. 2 = reluctant interaction. 3 = ready participation with peers. 4 = at ease with peers. 5 = initiates and maintains peer relationships. interaction with adults 1 = avoids contact. 2 = reluctant interaction. 3 = ready participation with peers. 4 = at ease with peers. / 5 = initiates and maintains peer relationship. figure 4. frequency ratings of categories of interactive behaviour. thesouth african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 49 (d) interaction — a correlation coefficient between interaction (with peers and adults) and imaginative play could not be determined due to the differing number of scores rated along the two dimensions. however, imaginative play has been viewed as a vehicle to enhance social development. g e n e r a l discussion the results demonstrated a linguistic deviation from the norm and a developmental delay in symbolic play, with sensori-motor activity predominating. thus, in relation to the aims of this study, s manifested with a breakdown in both representational functions but the nature of the deficit differed. the contention that the sensori-motor child is 'egocentric' in his overt actions while the pre-operational child is 'egocentric' in his representations,2 3 lends support to the finding that s is in a transition period from sensori-motor to symbolic activity. the symbolic function, rather than appearing in its final form, can be seen to build gradually upon sensori-motor achievements.3' 1 7 ' 2 6 ' the play protocols affirm that this child's, use of language was 'amorphous', similar to that of a two-year o l d . 1 8 it was almost exclusively an adjunct to direct ongoing activity, for example, ί riding bike!'. in addition, s made use of verbalizations to indicate his needs and as a tool to gain another person's attention. on the few occasions that s played symbolically in the unstructured situation it was the accompanying language, rather than the behavioural organization per se, that demonstrated its symbolism. this is contrary to the finding of lovell, hoyle and siddall1 5 that normal three and four-year olds gain so much satisfaction out of their self-created symbols in play that language is secondarily important. it is possible that s's inability to organize his play behaviours necessitated the use of language to explain its symbolism. only once, when he used a block of wood as a 'gun' to 'shoot' the gardener, did s demonstrate a substantial amount of pretending (a score of 4). his total involvement in this play activity was demonstrated by his frowning (suggesting anxiety) when the gardener fell down feigning injury, and by his 'seeking protection' behind an observer when the gardener 'aimed' the 'gun' at him. this seems to suggest that s has the 'competence' with regard to using play materials to transcend their immediate properties. however, performance would reflect this competence under ideal conditions only.5 in addition, variables such as the influence of television, suggest that this might be an imitative rather than a creative activity. s engaged predominantly in 'sensori-motor activity characterized by the mere pleasure of mastering reality'. his coincident verbalizations such as ί riding bike!' were bound to his overt activity. if he had referred to the bicycle as a space-ship, for example, clear transcendence of the immediate situation would have been demonstrated. die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 denise segal s's awareness of the 'double function' of objects and actions, (p. 191)9 and the ability to ascribe these actions to others as opposed to performing them only on himself, was apparent in his few symbolic activities. he rolled a 'snake' from dough and used it symbolically, warning the teacher that it is 'gonna bite you!' (representational stage — types 1a and ib). s also utilized language to symbolically identify one object with another, as when he picked up sand, threw it into the air and called it 'fire'. however, this cannot be considered as evidence of substage ii since s interposed physical action between himself and the represented object. as occurs in substage i, the 'psychological distance' (p. 328) 3 6 between the symbolizer and the symbolized was minimal. s's performance was found to be more advanced in the structured situation as compared with the unstructured setting. this is felt to be important in devising a therapy programme since both the child's potential and his performance should be taken into consideration. it is possible that restricting stimulus materials enhances awareness of novel symbolic applications to the play materials.1 7 however, according to piaget 2 4 who placed representational substage iii as occurring from age three, s is still functioning below his age level. in addition, only delay from the normal developmental sequence was evident contrary to impairment found for language development. the consideration of play within a cognitive-affective framework3 3 would account for s's maximal positive affect accompanying sensorimotor as opposed to symbolic behaviour. this model dictates that novel material, within the child's capacity for mastery, will yield interest, alertness and positive emotional reaction. as was evident from s's scores for organization of behaviour (fig. 2), he engaged most frequently in sensori-motor activity. since no cause-effect relationship has been determined, it is possible that improved concentration might result in improved imaginative play. in accordance with freyberg's8 suggestion, the present investigator feels that the two correlates may be a unitary dimension, the very nature of imaginative play implying the ability to concentrate. the present findings for .aggression accord with those of singer.3 3 freyberg8 did find however, that increased role-play behaviour results in decreased fighting. socialization with peers was often imitative and s seemed unable to initiate his own plan of action. he would play alone in their vicinity and subsequently emulate their behaviours particularly if these had been approved by an adult. the development of the symbolic function within the context of imitation as well as the apparent satisfaction s found in activities with adults, are both pertinent issues. hyperactivity, distractibility and perseveration, all evident in s's performance, have been found in association with languageimpairment. 3 4 ' 3 5 while the exact relationship between these behavioural manifestations, language and play is as yet undetermined, the thesouth african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 51 present findings demonstrated a positive correlation between concentration and imaginative play. thus, a therapy programme including symbolic play within a linguistic framework would necessarily be incorporating concentration whether directly or indirectly. clinical implications while a subjective, case study analysis does not allow for generalization of findings to other language-impaired children, it does exemplify aspects of diagnosis and therapy for these children. it is suggested that diagnosis of the child with a language difficulty should include an analysis of his level of play. this affords a non-verbal assessment of his cognitive abilities and general level of symbolic functioning as well as providing for therapeutic direction. for the child whose receptive language appears to be intact, the structured play situation can be utilized. this allows for a rapid assessment (about 20 minutes per child) involving clinical presentation of tasks graded developmental^. however, since verbal instructions are inherent in its design, it is unsuitable for the child with severe receptive language impairment or for the profoundly deaf child. it is suggested that these latter children be observed in an unstructured setting, either at home or at nursery school and rated along a scale such as that devised by lunzer. this is time-consuming but it will facilitate differential diagnosis with regard to the child's general level of symbolic functioning. it is felt that language therapy should be carried out in relation to the child's total cognitive functioning. whether cognition can be taught is controversial, but materials and activities which are high relative to the child's present level of mastery should be provided in order to challenge his intellectual growth. 1 1 thus, as with regard to his language, so his play must be adapted to the child's unique stage of symbolic usage. the present writer proposed two ways in which symbolic play can be useful in language therapy: 1. for the child who is using only one-three word utterances — since the child learns language in relation to his ongoing activities as he interacts with the environment, parents should be encouraged to verbalize their own and the child's actions to allow for word-action association. from the naming of real objects, the child should progress to reproducing the same action using realistic toys and then 'junk' material, following the developmental sequence. daily activities can be re-enacted in 'pretend' play, for example, acting out a situation of 'going shopping' after the child's return from such an outing. at a later stage, the outing can be played out prior to its occurrence in real life. increased play complexity involves progressive 'deceleration' of the action from the child's own body, and complexity both in the use of the play material and in the behaviour (or behavioural die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 denise segal sequence) itself. with increased complexity so the time spent in the activity will be increased, thereby encouraging longer periods of concentration. in addition, involvement in the play activity will lead to positive affect during play, which in turn, will motivate further play. aggression can also be expressed within the play situation rather than being directed outward. 2. for the child who has acquired sufficient syntax — it is proposed that symbolic play should be embodied within the linguistic approach of interactive language development teaching devised by lee et a l . 1 4 to replace the use of a flannel board, objects such as boxes, sticks etc. can be used. these should be kept out of sight and brought into the child's focus only when mentioned in the story, to prevent excessive hyperactivity. abstract uses for these objects in relation to the ongoing narrative should be suggested and the child should be encouraged to discover additional symbolic applications. as the story is related and the child asked to respond to the clinician's utterances, so he must simultaneously utilize the objects to create the scene. for example, if the theme is a fishing trip, a cup could be a container for worms, pieces of paper could be worms, and so on. in this approach, the child can actively interact with is environment, an important consideration in intellectual growth. 4 , n ' 2 7 , 2 9 he is encouraged to explore his surroundings and to discover things for himself. the child who is hyperactive can draw the scene as it is related rather than acting it out or reproducing it in miniature. the flexibility of this method allows for other approaches to be incorporated within it. an example is the 'forced alternative' questioning put forward in the remediation technique of crystal et a l . 6 this can be used together with a story devised by lee et a l ; 1 4 a story made up by either the clinician or the child; as well as a symbolic sequence enacted or drawn. particular emphasis can be placed on that area (syntactic, semantic, categorical, etc.) most delayed for the child. in addition, most of these play activities can be carried out in groups which allows for socialization, interaction and co-operation, parallel play and imitation, all of importance for both linguistic and cognitive development. in conclusion, future research should be geared towards normative studies in this field which may highlight theoretical controversies, and towards evaluating therapy programmes which have a representational basis comprising'both language and symbolic play. references 1. bass, k., brown, j. b. & redmond, a. p. (1975): symbolic play in normal and language-impaired children. unpublished master of science dissertation, department of speech pathology and audiology, emerson college, canada. thesouth african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 53 2. bloom, l. (1970): language development: form and function in emerging grammars. m.i.t. press, massachusetts. 3 bowerman, m. f. (1974): discussion summary — development of concepts underlying language. chap. 7 in language perspectives — acquisition, retardation and intervention, schieffelbusch, r. l. and lloyd, l. l. (eds.), university park press, u.s.a. , . 4. bricker, w. a . & bricker, d. .s. (1974): an early language training strategy. chap. 17 in language perspectives — acquisition, retardation and interaction, schieffelbusch, r. l. and lloyd, l l. (eds.), university park press, u.s.a. 5. chomsky, n. (1975): aspects of the theory of syntax. m.i.t. press, massachusetts. 6 crystal, d . , fletcher, p. & garman, m. (1976): the grammatical analysis of language disability. edward arnold publishers, london. d 1 f 7 el'konin d (1971): symbolics and its functions in the flay ot children.' chap. 14 in child's play, herron, w. e. and sutton-smith, b., (eds.), john wiley and sons, inc., new york. 8 freyberg, j. (1973): increasing the imaginative play of urban disadvantaged kindergarten children through systematic training. chap. 6 in the child's world of make-believe — experimental studies of imaginative play, singer, j. l., (ed.), academic press, inc., new york. 9. furth, h. g. (1966): thinking without language: psychological implications of deafness. the free press, new york. 10 furth h. g. (1975): on the nature of language from the perspective of research with profoundly deaf children. annals of the new york academy of science, 263, 70-75. 11 furth, h. g. & wachs, h. (1974): thinking goes to school: piaget's theory in practice. oxford university press, inc., u.s.a. 12. hulme, j. & lunzer, e. a. (1966): play, language and reasoning in subnormal children. / . child psychol. psychiat., 7, 107-123. 13. lee, l. l. (1974): developmental sentence analysis. northwestern university press. evanston, illinois. 14. lee, l. l., koenigsknecht, r. a. & mulhern, s. (1975): interactive language development teaching: the clinical presentation of grammatical structure. northwestern university press, evanston, illinois. 15. lovell, k., hoyle, h. w. & siddall, m. q. a study of some aspects of the play and language of young children with delayed speech. / . child psychol. psychiat., 9, 41-50. 16. lowenfeld, m. (1939): the world pictures of children. british journal of medical psychology, 18, 65-101. 17. lunzer, e. a . (1959): intellectual development in the play of young children. educational review, 11, 205-217. die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 denise segal 18. luria, a. r. & yudovich, f. la."(1959): speech and development of mental processes in the child. penguin books, ltd., england. 19. moerk, e. l. (1977): pragmatic and semantic aspects of early language development. university park press, u.s.a. 20. moore, τ. e. & harris, a. e. (1978): learing and thought in piagetian theory. chap. 6 in alternatives to piaget: critical essays on the theory, siegal, l. s. and brainard, c. j., (eds.), academic press, new york. 21. morehead, d. m. (1972): early grammatical and semantic relations: some implications for a general representational deficit in linguistically deviant children. in papers and reports on child language development, ingram, d., (ed.), stanford university, stanford. 22. morehead, d. m. & ingram, d. (1973): the development of base syntax in normal and linguistically-deviant children. j. speech hearing res., 16, 330-344. 23. phillips, j. l. (1969): the origins of intellect: piaget's theory. w. h. freeman and co., san francisco. 24. piaget, j. (1951): play, dreams and imitation in childhood. heinemann, london. 25i piaget, j. (1966): response to brian sutton-smith. chap. 22 in child's play, herron, r. and sutton-smith, b. (eds.), john wiley and sons, inc., new york, 1971. 26. piaget, j. (1967): six psychological studies. university of london press, london. 27. piaget, j. (1971): the construction of reality in the child. ballantine books, inc., new york. 28. piaget, j. & inhelder, b. (1969): the psychology of the child. routledge and kegan paul, london. 29. riley, s. s. (1973): some reflections on the value of children's play. young children, 146-153. 30. rubin, κ. h. & maioni, t. l. (1975): play preference and its relationship to egocentrism, popularity and classification skills in pre-schoolers. merrill-palmer quarterly, 21, 3, 171-179. 31. sinclair, h . (1970): the transition from sensori-motor behaviour to symbolic activity. interchange, 1, 3, 119-126. 32. sinclair-de-zwart, h. (1972): a possible theory of .language acquisition within the general framework of piaget's developmental theory. chap. 19 in language in thinking, adams, p., (ed.), penguin books, ltd., england. 33. singer, j. l. (1973): the child's world of make-believe experimental studies of imaginative play. academic press, inc., new york. 1 34. strauss, a. a. & kephart, n. c: (1955): psychopathology and education of the brain-injured child. vol. ii. grune and stratton, new york. the-south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) verbal language and symbolic play 5 5 35 strauss, a. a. & lehtinen, l. (1950): psychopathology and ' education of the brain-injured child. grune and stratton, n.y 36 werner, h. & kaplan, b. (1963): symbol formation: an organismic-developmental approach to language and the expansion of thought. john wiley and sons, inc., n.y. die suid-afrikaanse t y d s k r i f vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) photographic teaching materials visual aids for use in language development catalogue and price list available from: p . t . m . , 23, horn steet, winslow, buckingham. mk18 3ap. england. tel: winslow (029 671) 3776. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) society of hearing aid consultants (s.a.) the society of hearing aid consultants (sa.) is a group of concerned hearing aid suppliers dedicated to improving and maintaining standards of hearing aid dispensing. our members have freely committed themselves to abide by the rules and disciplines of the society in the interests of promoting the highest standards of conduct and competence. special membership is available to interested persons in allied fields. enquiries: the secretary, society of hearing aid consultants (sa), po box 4581 johannesburg 2000 tel: 23-5791 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) operation of the auditory feedback monitoring loop in children with articulatory defects sandra o s s i p , b . a . l o g . ( r a n d ) introduction audition serves as the principle controlling unit in the speech servosystem, the motor aspect being subject in a large part, to the command of audition. from studies carried out on the deaf and hard-of-hearing population, such as that of carr,1 it is apparent that although normal hearing and auditory defective infants have many similarities in their early vocalizations, they develop aberrant speech patterns. chase3 quoted othei types of clinical cases, such as those with somesthetic sensory deficits, and concluded that both acoustic information about speech and intact feedback are relevant in the normal acquisition of speech, and lead, according to chases' hypothesis, to neurophysiological organization which underlies the ability to speak. the motor and sensory systems, and the interaction between the two, are necessary for the development of verbal behaviour. a closed-loop control system exists when the system can operate to control the machine of which it is a part, and is error-sensitive, error-measuring, self-adjusting and goal directed. in contrast, an openloop system would not be able to measure the output and make adjustments, if the results are not those desired. mysak8 was one of the first to hypothesize the development of closed internal and external audiovocal and audioverbal loops as the components of the speech system. in this study project the writer is concerned with the internal monitoring feedback loop, but information about the development of this, particularly the auditory feedback monitoring system for speech, is rather scant. the studies carried out to date have assumed, as fairbanks4 did, that the closed-loop auditory feedback monitory system can be inferred from the disturbances in speech produced by an experimental technique which delays the air conducted feedback to the speaker's ears, i.e. delayed auditory feedback (da'f). studies done by yeni-komshian,12 chase,3 mackay7 and chase et al.2 suggest that older children show greater daf effects than younger children, indicating that in younger children, the monitoring loop is still in the process of being closed and the motor organization of speech incomplete or still under practice. ; when considering the importance of sensation in this loop system and in the consequent acquisition of speech, disregarding the motor mechajournal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 18, december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a u d i t o y feedback monitoring l o o p in articulatory defects 49 nisms which may be inadequate, defective speech could arise from a defective model which guides the output, or, from a failure of this mechanism which compares the output with the intended or desired production. within the cybernetic framework adopted here, aspects such as auditory memory span and phonetic discrimination defects can be considered as "substandard functioning of some component of the speech servo-mechanism" which may be possible because of many functional articulatory disorders.5 here it is assumed that the defect arises from a failure of the comparative mechanism from which the original auditory image or model had to be derived. because of the significance of maturation of the ability to articulate, another aspect must be considered. this is the possibility that the loop control system in children with articulation disorders, has not been closed and does not have the properties of "monitoring" the output. experimental methodology aim the purpose of this experiment is to determine whether the auditory feedback monitoring system for speech is operative in children with multiple articulatory errors. this assessment was done by mechanically inducing an interference in the air conducted loop and studying the effects on a sample of the above-mentioned population as compared to the effects on a normal population-sample. the hypothesis thus being that the children with articulatory disorders will show less severe breakdown under delayed auditory feedback than children with no articulatory defects. method i. subjects the ages of the subjects ranged from four years seven months to nine years five months; six of the children were males and four females. the experimental group consisted of three males and two females who were known to have two or more articulatory errors. the control group consisted of five subjects who were pair matched with the experimental group on the following variables: (a) sex. (b) age.—the subjects were matched to have equal ages within approximately three months. (c) educational level.—the controls were taken from the same classes as the experimental subjects. the subjects taken from a nursery school were in the same group. (d) intelligence.—subjects were not formally tested and matched, but were judged to be "above average" or "average" by the individual class teachers. (e) level of maturity.—assessed and judged by the class teachers. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. is, desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50, sandra ossip (f) personality.—no formal tests were administered but children were matched according to teachers' recommendations. (g) socio-economic status.—the children all attended schools which were situated in similar types of areas. they could be assessed to be of middle socio-economic level. (h) aircfiildren had histories of normal speech and hearing development, and apparently normal central nervous systems. 2. instrumentation (a) an amplivox audiometer (model 82) was used to establish the air-conduction threshold for each subject. (b) a national tape recorder (model rq7065) was used to record all responses made while the subjects were in the experimental situation, and these samples were used for later analysis. the speed for recording was kept constant at i.p.s. and recording volume level also remained fixed. (c) the madsen (model bs63) delay speech unit was used to induce the experimental conditions of normal auditory feedback (naf) and delayed auditory feedback (daf). the recorder and playback heads were combined, enabling adjustment of delay intervals. the critical interval of 0,2 seconds was used throughout the experiment. the output intensity level was also kept constant. maico soft cushion headphones were used. 3. materials (a) the templin darley picture articulation test was used and the spontaneous method of eliciting responses was followed. validity and reliability of the examiner's judgements of articulation were found to be adequate, following appropriate statistical and subjective analysis. two other therapists independently recorded the same responses. (b) the material used for two of the three tasks in the experiment consisted of two sets of five cards each (set a and set b). the cards .contained clear pictures of common every-day objects together with a clearly written representation of them. (c) recitation of a nursery rhyme. 4. procedure the experimentation took place in an iac (series 1600 act) soundproof testing booth where the experimenter established' an air conduction threshold for each ear. the articulation screening test was then administered to assess the subjects' articulatory abilities. after a rest interval, the subject was taken to another soundproof room where the child was settled with the experimenter e. the microphone of the tape recorder was placed on the table so that it was able to pick up all the responses elicited from the child. the earphones, which were connected to the delayed speech 'unit, were placed over the child's ears. ε presented the first set of cards (set a) within the child's visual journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. is, december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a u d i t o y feedback monitoring l o o p in articulatory defects 51 field and the subject was asked to name the pictures on the cards. when this was completed to the satisfaction of the e, the child was asked to repeat the naming procedure from the beginning. before the following repetition was commenced, the child was rold of the possibility that his voice "may sound funny". the daf condition was then commenced. as soon as the child completed the task of naming all five cards, the daf condition was discontinued. the same procedure was repeated with the second set of cards (set b) first under the condition of naf and then daf. the final experimental task was then begun, where the subject was required to repeat a nursery rhyme under the two conditions of naf and daf. results analysis after a short pilot study conducted on ten adult subjects under naf and daf conditions, and critical evaluation of the information gained on these subjects, the writer felt that the measures which would yield empirical data that could be subjected to analysis would be: 1. time: the mean rate reduction score was obtained by subtracting the time taken for a task under naf from the time taken under daf. three such scores were obtained for each subject, one for each task. 2. the second score was a more qualitative measure of the number of words prolonged under daf. the experimenter judged these, after finding that two judges' (speech therapists) evaluations agreed with her's. a percentage score was obtained for each subject on the two sets of cards. percentage of words prolonged = number of words prolonged x 100. 3. observation of the subjects during the experimental situation. their reactions and verbal responses were recorded by the experimenter. method of analysis 1. the time scores (in seconds) were recorded and summaries were compiled allowing for comparison between the experimental and control groups. 2. the time scores obtained for task three (the nursery rhyme) were subjected to a test of significance. the student "t" test was applied to see if the differences between the two groups were significant. 3. the mean percentage prolongation scores obtained for each group were subjected to a similar "t" test analysis. 4. a graph was drawn to represent the age-link hypothesis. the percentage prolongation of the subjects in the experimental group was used, where each subject represented a different age level. discussion under conditions of daf, all the "normal" subjects or those having no articulatory errors, broke down considerably in comparison to their tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. , desember r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 sandra ossip performance under naf. in contrast, under similar conditions, the experimental group showed less impairment of their natural speech and voice. the most marked change in speech, was the increase in time taken to produce the same speech sample under daf than under naf. these scores, together with the mean prolongation scores (tables 1 and 2) revealed that there were significant differences between the two samples of the population at the 15% level of confidence. subjects reduction score control group i reduction score experimental group ii difference between reduction score ( / / / ) pair 1: (ca = 4.6 years) ci 38 el 4 34 pair 2: (ca = 6.3 years) c2 22 e2 19 3 pair 3: (ca = 6.7 years) c3 28 e3 13 15 pair 4: (ca = 7.5 years) c4 5 e4 11 6 pair 5: (ca = 9 . 5 years) c5 14 e5 10 4 significant at the .001 % level. table 1: summary of the mean reduction scores of task three subjects number of prolongations control group i number of prolongations experimental group ii difference between prolongations ( / / / ) pair 1: (ca = 4.6 years) ci 7 el 0 7 pair 2: (ca = 6.3 years) c2 4 e2 2 2 pair 3: (ca = 6.7 years) c3 2 e3 2 0 pair 4: (ca = 7.5 years) c4 3 e4 3 0 pair 5: (ca = 9.5 years) c5 6 e5 3 3 significant at the 10% level. table ii: summary of the mean prolongation scores on tasks one and two the results indicate that the auditory feedback monitoring loop system is not operating at the same level or is still in the process of being "closed" in the child with functional articulatory defects compared to the child with normal speech. it appears that chase's2 hypothesis, which states that the auditory feedback monitoring system for speech develops as a function of age, journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. is, december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a u d i t o y feedback monitoring l o o p in articulatory defects 53 is borne out in these results (see figure 1). it was noted that a subject of four years shows less marked behavioural disturbance under daf figure: the percentage of breakdown in the experimental group, measured by prolongations as a function of age. it can thus be assumed that the speech system develops from an open 'external audio-vocal loop to form a closed audio-verbal loop. this is the most complex form of behaviour as it is the ability to monitor, to check the output and to change it if necessary. under daf, the sensory information and the motor output are out of phase, and this results in a disruption of speech, as there is an interruption in the temporal pattern of motion. if a person regulates his behaviour continuously, there will be a marked effect on his speech under daf. if, however, the temporal patterning is not as organized or practiced, the disruption will not be as severe, as the loop is not a closed monitoring circuit thus the child with articulatory disorders appears to be immature in the development and acquisition of speech. it appears from this project that sensory information is as relevant to speech development as is the motor aspect. auditory feedback consequently appears to play an important role in the process of speech acquisition. the writer has in this study continually stressed the importance of sensory feedback for adequate motor function, but it would be an oversimplification to say that the organism functions as a computer, and treat him as a simple control system. she has assumed the basic hypothesis that the individual possesses the control properties of a cybernetic system, but as a living system, exhibits flexibility and change in pattern of control. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. is, desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 sandra ossip one of the most complex forms of behaviour is verbal communication, which emerges in the context of active exchange of acoustic information that ultimately results in the development of a system of speech motor gestures that conforms to the system utilized by the adult culture. some therapeutic procedures have been devised by therapists such as mysak8 and van riper and irwin11, but the writer feels that not enough emphasis is placed on audition as being the major source of sensory feedback information. she advocates use of increased auditory stimulation, auditory training techniques, and the use of the auditory training aid (train-ear) in order to develop the auditory feedback monitoring system for speech. the therapist, at first, is the monitor, and eventually the child can exercise automatic control. in a similar manner other speech disorders can be attributed to breaks in the cybernetic feedback systems and therapy programmes be devised. an implication of the above theorising that has not been fully discussed is the classification of function in the structure of the nervous system. one assumption that is important is that the stream of speech is activated and controlled by a multiple determined plan and intention of communication, by antecedent and simultaneous internal and external events, by intention and organization of the message and by further sets of instructions that activate and control the movements that convert the message into sound. this assumption leads to an auxiliary statement that disorganization of the motor aspects of speech behaviour results from a variety of lesions. aphasia results from interference that disrupts both analysis and integration of verbal messages as the analysis and integration of language requires continuous and dynamic discriminatory and feedback activity. schuell et al.10 advocated her aphasic therapy on the above assumptions. her principles stress the need to provide the patient with adequate feedback by giving him adequate and increased stimulation. the patient depends on the therapist or a machine for feedback until he is able to regulate his behaviour in terms of his own feedback. mecham6 considered apraxia with this framework. apraxia refers to a disorder in the motor functioning in which "the individual has a basic motor capability to perform a motor act, but is unable to plan the act at an identical level, or transpose a visual impression into an appropriate motor counterpart or sequence". he viewed apraxia as a "distorted or unstable or nonvaried reafferent feedback''and the disruption of visual perceptual function in terms of failures or retardation of central organization". the training of these children and other braininjured children is orientated towards facilitating basic processes of general neurological organization, establishing or enhancing intermodal sensory integration, and to help clarify or define the child's sensory motor interaction. the child with: "functional articulatory errors" can be seen to have a similar defect though there is no known central nervous system involvement. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. , december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) auditoi-y feedback monitoring l o o p in articulatory defects 55 implications from this can be extended to problems encountered in teaching speech to the deaf child. here there is definite auditory sensory deprivation. the writer feels that it" is necessary not only to provide the child with a complete representation of the adult speech models to allow them to be reproduced, but it is also necessary to provide this information in a form that allows the child to match his speech with the adult speech, and compute the degree of approximation necessary to make systematic alterations directed at minimizing the mismatch between the two. the cybernetic interpretation of the developmental literature, taking into account the closed-loop nature of the behaviour and its selfregulating characteristics, suggests some ideas about teaching and training in general. the two main principles developing are: that the individual at all ages should be aided in gaining control over his own actions, and that teaching of specific skills must be adjusted to the phase of development of the feedback-control mechanism. teaching and training design should be adjusted to the level of control already achieved and also should be sensitive to the potential changes in response organization that may come in time through maturation. summary this study constitutes a preliminary evaluation of the utilization of auditory feedback for the acquisition of normal speech in normal speaking children and children having functional articulatory errors. the degree to which this is utilized for the organization and control of motor activity was inferred by delaying auditory feedback in time and quantitating the resulting disturbances in the speech behaviour. evidence was found to support the following hypotheses: i. there is a breakdown of speech under daf. ϊ : children with multiple articulatory disorders exhibit less severe breakdown effects under daf than their normal peers. 3. there appears to be a strong relationship between increasing age and articulatory ability. 4. there tends to be a relationship between increasing age and the breakdown of speech under daf. 5. monitoring of speech is a highly skilled control system which tends to develop with age and experience, and is not operating as strongly in the child with articulation-defects. from the results of the study, it seems that the auditory feedback monitoring loop for speech is not operating as successfully in the child with multiple articulatory errors as it operates in the normal child, and that the development of a closed loop system appears to be retarded in some way. opsomming hierdie studie vorm 'n voorbereidende evaluasie van die beniitting van ouditiewe terugvoering vir die verwerwing van spraak in normaaltydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. , desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sandra ossip sprekende kinders en kinders met funksionele artikulasiefoute. bewyse is verkry om die volgende hipoteses te o n d e r s t e u n : 1. d a a r is 'n afbraak van spraak tydens vertraagde ouditiewe terugvoering. 2. kinders met veelvoudige artikulasieafwykings vertoon minder ernstige afbraakeffekte tydens vertraagde ouditiewe terugvoering as hul normale maats. 3. d a a r blyk 'n sterk verhouding te wees tussen ouderdomsverhoging en artikulatoriese vermoe. 4. d a a r is ' n neiging tot 'n verhouding tussen ouderdomsverhoging en die afbraak van spraak tydens vertraagde ouditiewe terugvoering. 5. aktiewe kontrolering van spraak is 'n hoogs ontwikkelde sisteem wat neig om te ontwikkel met o u d e r d o m en ondervinding. dit blyk ook nie so aktief te wees in die kind met artikulasieafwykings nie. uit die resultate verkry van hierdie studie wil dit voorkom asof die kontrolebaan van die ouditiewe terugvoering van spraak nie so effektief werkende is in die kind m e t veelvoudige artikulasie foute as in die n o r m a l e kind van dieselfde o u d e r d o m nie. die ontwikkeling van 'n geslote baansisteem blyk o p een of ander wyse vertraag te wees. r e f e r e n c e s 1 carr j (1953): an investigation of the spontaneous speech sounds of the five-year-old deaf born child. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 18, 22-29 2 chase, r. α., sutton, s., first, b. and zubin, j. a. (1961): a developmental study of changes in behaviour under delayed auditory feedback. journal of genetic psychology, 99, 101-112. 3 chase, r. a. (1967): motor organization of speech. annual report. johns hopkins university school of medicine, baltimore, maryland, 103-145. 4. fairbanks, g. (1955): selective effects of delayed auditory feedback. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 20, 333-345. 5 mange, c. n. (1960): relationship between selected auditory perceptual factors' and articulation ability. journal of speech and hearing research, 6 mecham, m. j. (1966): introduction—nature and scope of the problem in communication training in childhood brain injury. charles c. thomas. 7. mackay, d. g. (1968): metamorphosis of a critical interval: age-linked changes in the delay in auditory feedback that produces maximal disruption of speech. journal of the acoustic society of america, 43, 811-821. 8. mysak, e. d. (1966): speech pathology and feedback theory. charles c. thomas. springfield, illinois. . 9. reef, h. (1968): lecture notes in "neurology given to speech and hearing therapy students. , 10. schuell, h., jenkins, j. j. and jimenez-pabon, e. (1967): aphasia in adults. harper and row, inc., new york.. ' 11. van riper, c. and irwin, j. n. (1959): voice and articulation. pitman medical publishing co., london. 12. yeni-komshian, g. (1968): the development of auditory feedback monitoring : no. 2. delayed auditory feedback studies on the speech of children between 2 and 3 years of age. journal of speech and hearing research, 11, 307-315. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. , december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aphasia: a societal and clinical appraisal of pragmatic and linguistic behaviours glenn m. goldblum, ma (sp. & h.sc.) (uc, santa barbara) department of speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract pragmatic abilities of eleven aphasics classified into fluent and nonfluent groups were examined, and compared with global ratings of communicative adequacy. further, subjects 'pragmatic performance was compared with performance on two standardised linguistic measures. all subjects demonstrated high levels of appropriate pragmatic behaviours and were apparently resourceful in using the context, frequently facilitated by use of compensatory communicative strategies. similar and different pragmatic deficits were noted for both groups. regardless of classification and linguistic severity, subjects were less impaired on pragmatic compared to standardised linguistic measures. implications were highlighted, emphasising the importance of pragmatics relative to other aspects of language function. opsomming die pragmatiese vermoens van elf afasie-pasiente, wat ingedeel is in 'n vlot — en 'n onvlotgroep, is ondersoek en hul kommunikatiewe vaardighede is onderling vergelyk. die proefpersone se pragmatiese prestasie is ook vergelyk met hul prestasie op twee gestandaardiseerde linguistiese toetse. al die proefpersone het bewys gelewer van hoe vlakke van toepaslike pragmatiese gedrag en was oenskynlik vindingryk om die konteks te gebruik vir kompensatoriese kommunikatiewe strategiee. tekortkominge in die twee groepe was soms dieselfde en soms verskillend van aard. ongeag die klassifikasie van die groepe en die graad van linguistiese onvermoe, was die proefpersone deurgaans pragmaties minder belemmer as op die linguistiese vlak. gevolgtrekkings is beklemtoon, wat die relatiewe belang van die pragmatiek tot ander aspekte van taalfunksionering onderstreep. as speech-language pathologists, one of our primary concerns has always been to improve communicative effectiveness of the individuals we serve. in attempting to meet this goal, the intervention strategies we have used have been continually refined and revised-molded by the changing theoretical views of language over the past several decades. earlier in our history many investigators including johnson (1946) and van riper (1939 cited by prutting, 1982a) stressed the need for our goals to be mutually acceptable to both the client and society. much of this societalj perspective was lost over the past few decades, perhaps in the name of objectivity and accountability. more recently, researchers in applied behavioural research and in the field of child and adult language disorders, (e.g. kazdin (1977); mueller (1983); prutting (1982a); prutting and kirchner (1983); and prutting, kirchner, hassan and buen (1984)) have pointed out the need to move back clinically to viewing communication disorders from a social perspective, taking context into account, rather than attempting to control it. despite this proposed change in perspective, clinical aphasiologists still need to concern themselves with identifying behaviours and effecting changes that make socially relevant differences in the client's life. as in the discipline of science, the field of acquired adult aphasia has undergone many paradigmatic shifts which have resulted in conceptual and methodological reorganisation. at present there is a contemporary and ongoing shift towards a functionalist paradigm which appears to provide promise of a different theoretical framework for examining and attempting to understand the aphasic communication problem. holland's observation (1977) that "aphasics probably communicate better than they talk" (p. 173), which views aphasia as a communication (rather than a language) problem, reflects this broadened functional perspective. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 a perusal of the more recent literature reveals that despite the growing interest towards pragmatics in aphasia, a diverse and large list of behaviours are included under the general rubric of pragmatics, in the absence of a comprehensive theoretical framework of natural aphasic communication. such a framework remains to be developed to help clinicians incorporate pragmatics more effectively into the treatment process. _ while most traditional and current assessments of aphasic language focus on the structural aspects of their verbal output, a few researchers have begun to be challenged by, and address the assessment of diverse communicative functions in aphasia. using a variety of communication assessment protocols and observational measures a few investigators including guilford and o'connor (1982); holland (1980, 1982); and penn (1983) have demonstrated generally superior pragmatic abilities with little correlation between traditional syntactic profiles and communicative performance. while their data has furthermore resulted in support for holland's belief (1982, 1983) in the preservation of communicative competence in aphasia, penn (1983) has warned that since the social context of communication is so redundant, we may be overestimating the aphasic's ability. communicative competence compared to linguistic competence may thus best be viewed as less impaired in aphasia rather than intact. the most comprehensively linguistic study evident in this regard appears to be that by penn (1983), who developed a profile of communicative appropriateness (pca) based upon a relatively comprehensive taxonomy derived from child language literature. penn used her pca (penn, 1983) and the language assessment remediation and screening procedure (larsp) (crystal, garman and fletcher, 1976 cited by penn, 1983) to obtain and compare com© sasha 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 municative and linguistic profiles of hem.sphere aphasic subject, termined by the boston d j ^ ; ^ 1 9 8 3 ) . t h e r e s u l t s e t • c i t e d b y p e n n ' 1 9 8 3 ) of the larsr syntactic j ^ ] e s s e r e x t e n t s e v e n . correlated c.osely ^ . ^ ^ " f ^ o n s t r a t e d essentially idiosynskills by indivtdua. subjects, whtah were not n e c e s s a r i l y related to subjects' capacities on a syntactic level severity and type distinctions did not always account for subject clusters on the pca (penn, 1983), particularly when individual scales were considered, although severity was postulated to relate more closely to appropriate communicative performance than site of lesion or type of aphasia as predicted by traditional tests of aphasia. though exploratory in nature, penn's data (1983) suggesting the relative independence of syntactic and pragmatic aspects of aphasia are extremely provocative, raising concern with two important issues. firstly they cast some doubt on the validity of traditional classification schemes and their ability to reflect functional communicative competence. alternatively, penn's data highlights the need to consider structural and functional data together to facilitate a more comprehensive evaluation of an aphasic's communicative competence. in contrast to these data, in the area of child language disorders, prutting et al, (1984) have recently demonstrated clearly different pragmatic profiles across normal, language and articulation disordered groups of children using a societally-based pragmatic protocol (prutting 1982b) inclusive of the behaviours discussed in the literature. their documentation of a relationship between pragmatic function and linguistic performance, (which contrasts with the limited data reported in the adult aphasic literature) may reflect prutting et al's (1984) use of societal criteria to evaluate the childrens' language use, using a protocol based upon a sound theoretical frameglen goldblum work (austin, 1962; searle, 1969 cited by prutting et al, 1984) to evaluate a range of pragmatic behaviours. furthermore, mueller's data (1983) revealing a strong relationship between this protocol and measures of societal judgements, highlighted the apparent value of this tool as a means of approaching the assessment of social competence. while prutting and kirchner (1983), have emphasised the necessity to gather and analyse data utilising methods which reflect advances in our theoretical knowledge, to date there appears to be no study in the field of aphasia utilising societal criteria to analyse the individual's pragmatic strengths and deficits across a range of pragmatic behaviours. as of yet we have no clear understanding of how pragmatic abilities and deficits stratify across different types of aphasic clients. furthermore, despite the increasing number of studies examining discrete pragmatic behaviours in aphasic adults, a marked paucity is nevertheless apparent of investigations evaluating pragmatic function in relationship to linguistic performance. the theoretical, clinical and research consequences of using a societal pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) in conjunction with traditional clinical measures are apparently far-reaching. not only would the effects of communicative behaviour in relation to societal values be considered, but attempts would be made to merge clinical and societal goals into our intervention programmes, enhancing the overall communicative competence of the aphasic individual. in view of these issues, this study examined the pragmatic abilities of eleven aphasic adults classified into fluent and non-fluent groups using the western aphasia battery (wab) (kertesz, 1980) taxonomy, and compared them with global ratings of communicative adequacy. furthermore, subjects' performance on a societal pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) was compared with performance on two clinical linguistic measures the wab (kertesz, 1980) and the communicative abilities in daily living (cadl) (holland, 1980). table 1 descriptive data for subjects used in the study subject sex age months post onset educational level premorbid occupation premorbid communicativeness rating on scale from 1-7* apraxia rating on scale from 0-7** dysarthria rating on scale from 0-7** classification of type of aphasia on western aphasia battery behavioural classification severity rating on western alphasia battery 1 μ 70 7 high school retired auto service manager 3 0 0 wernicke's fluent moderate i 2 μ 58 27 college retired navy test pilot 3 2 0 conduction fluent mildmoderate 3 f 51 8 10th grade laundromat assistant, bartender 4 0 0 conduction fluent mild i 4 μ 69 5 8th grade retired security guard 5 0 2 anomic fluent mild 5 f 51 7 high school retired legal secretary 3 0 0 anomic fluent mild 6 f 69 3 ma (art) retired general manager 1 0 0 anomic fluent mild 7 μ 67 9 incompleted ma retired military colonel 4 5 0 broca's non-fluent moderate . · 8 μ 68 300 8th grade retired carpet & tile salesman 2 2 0 broca's non-fluent mild ' 9 μ 65 40 high school retired plumber 2 3 0 broca's non-fluent mild 10 μ 54 16 college retired licensed land surveyor 2 3 0 broca's non-fluent mild 11 f 60 120 high school retired meter maid 2 0 0 broca's ·' non-fluent mild *key: 1 = superior communicativeness 7 = uncommunicative " k e y : 0 = absent 7 = severe the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aphasia : a societal and clinical appraisal of pragmatic and linguistic behaviours 1 3 sonal clinical experience and the literature (e.g. holland, 1980; 1983; penn, 1983) seemed to highlight their importance in aphasia. method subjects eleven english speaking aphasic adults were selected for this study a c c o r d i n g to certain criteria. these included confirmed presence of aphasia due to a completed, single, left hemisphere cerebrovascular accident, neurological stability and the absence of gross conc o m i t a n t problems. all aphasics were required to have absent to moderate symptoms of apraxia and/or dysarthria ranging from 0-5 onaseverity scaleof7(wertz, 1984). portions of the motor speech evaluation (wertz, weiss, kurtzke et al., 1978) and the spontaneous section of the wab (kertesz, 1980) were used to determine this. a representation of both fluent and non-fluent aphasics was required as determined by the classification on the fluency subtest of the wab (kertesz, 1980). since fluency appears to be one of the most important factors differentiating the aphasia types, and is a dimension that has been emphasised in more recent behavioural classifications of aphasia, the fluent : non-fluent dichotomy was selected in this study to facilitate comparison of pragmatic abilities in fluent : non-fluent aphasic subjects. global aphasics were excluded to control for severity of the sample examined (wertz, 1984). table 1 illustrates relevant case history information pertaining to the aphasics. pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) is a societal protocol which reflects the theoretical shift to place language within the context of socialisation (prutting, 1982a). in contrast to traditional measures, the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) examines the individual's pragmatic strengths and deficits within the conversational discourse, and across a variety of contexts. "the resulting configuration allows the clinician to evaluate pragmatic function in relation to linguistic performance" (prutting, et al., 1984, p.24). the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) has been developed over a four year period and pilot tested at the university of california, santa barbara speech and hearing centre (prutting, et al., 1984). it comprises a pool of 32 behaviours all known to be developed and used appropriately by children entering school, adolescents and adults. this tool was designed to be used while observing individuals engaged in spontaneous conversation during unstructured communicative interactions (prutting, et al., 1984). the pragmatic protocol^prutting, 1982b) was developed using the following criteria: theoretical framework, representative range of behaviours found in normal development, and inter et al., 1984). •investigator reliability (prutting, theoretical framework: the speech act theory proposed by austin (1962) and searle (1969)|(cited by prutting et al., 1984), constitutes the framework underlying this pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b). • " the 32 behaviours were organised within the following speech act categories. the utterance act includes 13 behaviours which form the verbal, non-verbal and paralinguistic aspects of production, characterising the "raw material" of the communicative act (prutting and kirchner, 1983). the prepositional act is comprised of 4 behaviours which define the linguistic dimensions of meaning. the 15 items comprising the illocutionary and perlocutionary acts constitute the reciprocal behaviours that regulate discourse between speakers. the illocutionary act represents the speaker's intention, while the perlocutionary act represents the speaker or listeners' effects. modification to the pragmatic protocol in addition, the investigator added two behaviours namely personal appearance and affect (under the utterance act), since perdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 32, 1985 scoring since the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) is a societal appraisal rather than a clinical appraisal, the judgement made is not whether the behaviour is correct or incorrect in a clinical sense, but rather if the behaviour is penalising or not. the investigator must decide whether society will penalise an individual for exhibiting a particular behaviour. thus, a behaviour may be incorrect but not necessarily judged as inappropriate (prutting, et al, 1984). the following guidelines were used to judge each of the behaviours listed on the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b): appropriate: behaviours are marked appropriate if they facilitate the communicative interaction, or are neutral. inappropriate: behaviours are judged inappropriate if they detract from the communicative exchange and penalise the individual. no opportunity to observe: when the evaluator has insufficient information to judge the behaviour as appropriate or inappropriate (prutting and kirchner, 1983). scale of overall communicative adequacy a 5 point subjective rating scale (derived from holland's protocol for adult aphasia (1982)) was used to obtain an estimate of the aphasics' and their partners' overall communicative adequacy. each rating would be converted into a percentage out of a total of 5 for comparison purposes. testing procedure and schedule preselection tests and standardised procedures were administered over two sessions. aphasic clients were seen individually in various facilities, or in their home environments. a constant task presentation order was maintained. firstly, portions of the motor speech evaluation (wertz, et al., 1978) and the oral portion of the wab (kertesz, 1980) were administered. during the second session the cadl (holland, 1980) was administered, and each aphasic was videotaped while conversing with a familiar comfortable partner of his/her choice with whom they had some shared history. while c o n v e r s a t i o n a l partner and topic were not specified, the affiliative nature of the relationship, and topic familiarity were held constant across all aphasics to facilitate and maximise conversational flow. results pragmatic protocol data while fluent aphasics demonstrated significantly superior pragmatic skills compared with non-fluent aphasics (see table 2), all individuals demonstrated a high level of appropriate pragmatic behaviours which manifested as retained social competence (see figure 1). table 2 between group comparison of percent appropriate pragmatic behaviours on pragmatic protocol in fluent and non-fluent groups fluent group (n = 6) non-fluent group (n=5) x range s.d. 86.27%* 79.41-94.11% 2.07 x range s.d. 75.88%* 61.76-85.29% 2.95 *t-test significance at ρ <0.05 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 τ — i — i — i — i — i 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 n u m b e r of inappropriate pragmatic behaviors figure 1 number of inappropriate pragmatic behaviours on pragmatic protocol for individual subjects within each group table 3 percentage of inappropriate pragmatic behaviours for fluent and non-fluent aphasics in each speech act category group utterance act propositional act elocutionary perlocutionary act fluent 11.1% 33.3% 11.1% non-fluent 10.6% 55% 29.3% glen goldblum aphasics were apparently resourceful in using the context, frequently facilitated via the use of compensatory communicative strategies. these included word finding strategies, appropriate repair and revision strategies, and a range of largely effective idiosyncratic strategies such as non-talking, to avoid communicative failure. the highest proportion of inappropriate pragmatic behaviour displayed by both groups was in the propositional act category. the non-fluent group demonstrated significantly greater difficulty on the perlocutionary/illocutionary act level, reflecting greater problems in managing the dyad (see table 3 and figure 2). examination of profiles of pragmatic deficits across fluent and nonfluent groups revealed interesting similarities and differences. firstly, the nearly comparable presence of the following four inappropriate pragmatic behaviours across fluent and non-fluent groups: specificity/accuracy (100%) in both groups, fluency (50%) and (60%); pause time (50%) and (80%); quantity/conciseness (60%) and (100%) across fluent and non-fluent groups respectively. it is noteworthy that while fluency was rank ordered third as compared to sixth for fluent and non-fluent groups respectively, more nonfluent aphasics (60%) were penalised for this behaviour than fluents (50%). generally, the prevalence of these four deficits across both fluent and non-fluent aphasics would seem to appropriately reflect the inherent problem of aphasia, irrespective of classification. in contrast to these similarities, two inappropriate pragmatic behaviours were very prominent in the non-fluent group, and either relatively rare or absent in the fluent group. these were word order (16%) and (100%); variety of speech acts (0%) and (100%) in the fluent and non-fluent groups respectively. these data clearly seem to differentiate the fluent from the non-fluent aphasics suggesting that rules of dyadic interaction are most difficult for the non-fluent aphasics in this investigation. in view of the presence of similar and different inappropriate pragmatic behaviours within the fluent and non-fluent groups used in this study, it is apparent that the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) both does and does not differentiate between these aphasic groups. ro ο •·-» ο <υ ω in ro . c a . ® < σ> τ * s aphasic type 1 wernicke's fluent 2 conduction fluent 3 conduction fluent 4 anomic fluent 5 anomic fluent 6 anomic fluent 7 broca's nonfluent 8 broca's nonfluent 9 broca's nonfluent 10 broca's nonfluent 11 broca's nonfluent group utterance act figure 2 inappropriate pragmatic behaviours within each speech act category on pragmatic protocol for individual subjects in fluent and non-fluent groups the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) societal and clinical appraisal of pragmatic and linguistic behaviours 15 comparison of performance on the pragmatic protocol and the scale of overall communicative adequacy performance on the scale of overall communicative adequacy (holland, 1982) revealed the same trend evidenced in the fluent aphasic groups' overall superior performance on the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) thereby substantiating the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) as a measure reflecting communicative competences (see table 4). despite the aphasic individuals' use of largely effective compensatory strategies to enhance communicative success, communicative burden on their respective partners was nevertheless high, resulting in their use of a variety of frequently facilitative strategies (e.g. probing, and encouraging the aphasics' use of strategies). these data highlighted the inherently interactive nature of the communicative process. table 4 mean communicative adequacy ratings for fluent and non-fluent groups and their partners on the scale of overall communicative adequacy the relatively small range of mean percentage scores on the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) for the fluent and non-fluent groups (79.41-94.11% and 61.76-85.29% respectively) as compared with the greater ranges of scores for these groups on the wab (kertesz, 1980) and the cadl (holland, 1980) (see figure 3) was interesting, yielding several implications. this reflected an overall higher group mean of appropriate pragmatic behaviours (social competence scores) across fluent and non-fluent groups irrespective of severity. further, since the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) is based on societal rather than clinical criteria (used by the wab (kertesz, 1980) and the cadl (holland, 1980), these overall higher scores may also reflect the discrepancies reported in the literature, i.e. related to societal and clinical ratings of aphasic communicative competence. computational correlation coefficients were performed in order to determine and compare the relationship between societal and clinical performance of (a) fluent and non-fluent aphasic individuals fluent group* non-fluent group* x = 3.5 (70%) range = 3-4 s.d. = 0.55 χ = 2.4 (48%) range = 1-3 s.d. = 0.89 partner** partner** x = 4.6 (92%) range = 4-5 s.d. = 0.52 x = 4 (80%) range = 2-5 s.d. = 1.22 key: 'rating of 1 = minimal communicative ability rating of 5 = normal communicative ability ••rating of 1 = nominal skill in communicating with aphasic rating of 5 = competence in communicating with aphasic correlation between pragmatic protocol scores and performance on the western aphasia battery and the communicative abilities in daily living in fluent and non-fluent aphasic individuals the relationship between societal'and clinical profiles was examined and'revealed some interesting trends. while significant differences were obtained between social competence scores of fluent and nonfluent aphasics on the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) (p<0.05), differences between these two groups on the clinical measures (namely the wab (kertesz, 1980) and the cadl (holland, 1980)) were found to be nonsignificant (p>0.05). (see table 5) 100 90 80 70 60 50 40 30 2 0 10 f nf pragmatic protocol aphasic group and measure f nf wab f nf cadl key: f = fluent group nf = non-fluent group figure 3 percentage group means and ranges of appropriate pragmatic behaviour on the pragmatic protocol, and correct responses on the western aphasia battery and communicative abilities in daily living. table 5 percentage group means, ranges and standard deviations «η the pragmatic protocol, the western aphasia battery and the communicative abilities in daily living *t-test significance at ρ <0.05 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 fluent group non-fluent group measure x range s.d. x range s.d. pragmatic protocol 86.26%* 79.41 %-94.11% 2.07 75.88%* 61.76-85.29% 2.95 wab 74.92% 46.48%-96.26% 18.99 64.56% 42.96%-78.57 % 13.64 cadl 81.25% 52.94%-97.79% 22.95 86.32% 72.79%-94.85% 11.72 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 glen goldblum separately and (b) combined (see table 6). the nonignificant correlation (for both fluent and non-fluent groups) between the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) and the wab (kertesz, 1980), t d the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) and the cadl (holland 1980) suggested that these tests are measuring different aspects of an individual's communicative competence, and would not necessarily predict his/her performance on one or other test. table 6 computational correlation coefficient among three measures of fluent and non-fluent aphasics' communicative abilities group pragmatic protocol + wab pragmatic protocol + cadl wab + cadl fluent 0.20 0.34 0.97* non-fluent 0.83 0.13 0.57 non-fluent minus subject 7 0.32 0.28 0.99* fluent + non-fluent combined 0.53 0.05 0.75 fluent + non-fluent combined minus subject 7 0.26 0.15 0.91* *t-test significance at ρ <0.05 in the fluent group, the cadl (holland, 1980) correlated significantly with the wab (kertesz, 1980) (s = 0.97) (p<0.05) reflecting the cadl's (holland, 1980) almost perfect ability to predict a client's communicative abilities in daily living (holland, 1980). while the non-fluent group revealed a non-significant correlation between the wab (kertesz, 1980) and cadl (holland, 1980) (s = 0.57) (p>0.05), removal of subject seven's extremely variable data reduced the disproportion and resulted in comparable correlational trends between the fluent and non-fluent groups (see table 6). it is apparent from these data, that the highest correlation exists between the wab (kertesz, 1980) and the cadl (holland, 1980) while performance on the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) would not necessarily predict performance on the cadl (holland, 1980) and the wab (kertesz, 1980). similar correlational trends occur when the three measures of communicative abilities are compared for the fluent and non-fluent groups combined, and when the 'outsider' subject seven is removed (see table 6). the overall consistent pattern of generally low non-significant correlation of the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) with the cadl (holland, 1980), and even more so with the wab (kertesz, 1980) (p>0.05) when fluent and non-fluent groups are examined separately, and combined (excluding subject seven's data) yield some interesting implications — most notably the apparent trustworthiness of the data based on the stability of data. thus it is evident that fluent and non-fluent aphasics perform consistently superiorily on societal as compared to clinical measures. while these data are based upon a small and heterogeneous sample, which may have tended to inflate the obtained correlation coefficients, their implications are provocative. 'discussion a pragmatic perspective of aphasia in accordance with several investigators (e.g., davis and wilcox, 1981, foldi, cicone and gardner, 1983; guilford and o'connor, 1982; holland, 1977, 1982, 1983; penn, 1983; wilcox, 1983) the results of this study revealed a high level of appropriate pragmatic behaviours manifesting as retained social competence across all aphasic subjects. generally despite their linguistic impairments, aphasics are apparently resourceful in using the context effectively in conversational interactions, frequently facilitated via the use of compensatory communicative strategies (including for example, simplification, humour, circumlocutions and interjections to maintain turns). these strategies reflecting the aphasics' attempts to readapt communicatively to their cerebral insult were found by penn (1983) to constitute retained communicative competence. while the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) was able to differentiate between the fluent and non-fluent group of aphasics on the basis of the almost exclusive presence of two inappropriate pragmatic behaviours in the non-fluent group, namely, word order and variety of speech acts, in view of the following factors, caution needs to be observed in concluding that this tool clearly differentiates between fluent and non-fluent aphasics: the combined presence of four inappropriate pragmatic behaviours across fluent and nonfluent groups (i.e., specificity/accuracy, fluency, pause time, and quantity/conciseness); as well as the scattered presence of pragmatic deficits across individual aphasics, irrespective of classification. these latter idiosyncratic deficits attest to the perplexing feature of variability that is so characteristic of aphasia (holland, 1983). furthermore, the small sample of fluent aphasics manifesting mild linguistic deficits combined with a relative absence of pragmatic deficits may well have inflated the data. further research using larger samples of equivalent types of aphasic subjects is clearly needed to clarify these issues. to the present writer's knowledge, penn's investigation comparing syntactic and pragmatic abilities of aphasics appears to be the one most closely related to the research questions addressed in this study. it is therefore interesting to speculate why penn's profile of communicative appropriateness (pca) (penn, 1983), unlike the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) did not reveal any clearly differentiating pragmatic deficits across aphasic types. features of the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) that may account for these differential findings appear to include the following: firstly, it encompasses a broad range of pragmatic behaviours which are examined in a more global or molar rather than a molecular manner. secondly, the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) employs societal criteria of appropriateness. furthermore the data base used in this study was spontaneous conversation as compared with penn's study (1983), where topic content was controlled. pragmatic protocol profiles and overall communicative adequacy ratings ; the contemporary revival of the awareness of the importance of subjectivity in science (gould, 1981), resulted in holland's ;scale of overall communicative adequacy being used in the present study in an effort to provide a global subjective rating of the aphasic individual's communicative adequacy with their partner. since all communicative partners used in this study were familiar and affiliative, the latter being an acknowledged factor contributing to communicative efficiency (linebaugh, kryzer, oden and myers 1982), it was apparent that overall, the manner and ease with which these partners communicated with the aphasic, and helped share the communicative burden, facilitated greater communicative success with less frustration in the dyad. more effective partners tended to use strategies such as slowing down, stress, cueing, and encouraging the aphasic's use of strategies (e.g. writing, repetition, and a variety of cues such as requesting help from the listener). this use of a range of compensatory strategies by the aphasic was likewise seen to be related to their overall superior performance on the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a societal and clinical appraisal of pragmatic and linguistic behaviours 17 aphasia the clinical implications of these data are apparently far reaching, yielding support for linebaugh, et al's suggestion (1982) that it would be valuable to help aphasics (within their limitations) and their partners maximise their share of the communicative burden, and increase the effective use of demonstrated compensatory strategies, thereby enhancing the efficiency of the communication in the dyad. social and clinical profiles while the fluent and non-fluent groups demonstrated significantly different pragmatic competencies, these inter group differences were not observed when comparing performance on the clinical profiles (namely the wab (kertesz, 1980) and the cadl (holland, 1980)). possible reasons accounting for the stable pattern of discrepant performance apparent across these pragmatic and linguistic tasks include differential task requirements and criteria used in the evaluation of each aphasic's performance. these data imply firstly that in the sample investigated, overall high pragmatic competence was apparently independent of linguistic severity, and secondly that these tools are therefore measuring different aspects of an individual's communicative competence. it is therefore apparent that severity can no longer be measured by linguistic competence alone, but that measures of pragmatic competence need to be considered in relation to these other aspects of language function. this latter interpretation was supported by the overall insignificant correlation between the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) and the wab (kertesz, 1980), and the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) and the cadl (holland, 1980) in fluent and non-fluent groups separately and combined. while level of linguistic severity was insignificant in the present study, closer examination of the data reveals some apparent contribution to communicative competence by severity. for example, examination of the data showing fluent and non-fluent group performance on the four subtests of the wab (kertesz, 1980) indicates overall superior linguistic performance for the fluent group as compared with the non-fluent group. since these aphasics are generally the least impaired (holland, 1980), the data again points toward a role played by linguistic severity in overall communicative competence. future research is needed to clarify these issues. / theoretical implications the findings of this study clearly demonstrated the aphasic individuals' retained pragmatic abilitiesinecessary for social competence. support was thereby rendered for holland's observation that "aphasics probably communicate better than they talk" (1977, p. 173). all aphasics' social competence scores were above the level predicted by their linguistic impairments. these data therefore indicate that despite linguistic limitations, relatively intact pragmatic abilities permit effective communication and management with others within the reality of the client's limitations. despite this, interpretation of pragmatic deficits independent of linguistic structure was problematic. as with the language disordered child (prutting, et al, 1984), it is evident that some 'spill over' exists between these different areas of communicative competence, and that the processes underlying appropriate use of pragmatic skills are not easily explained apart from the aphasic individual's linguistic limitations. however, overall superior pragmatic competence in relation to other linguistic skills may well account for the differential progress made by aphasic clients of apparently equal severity. while critical evaluation of traditional theories and methodologies through the telescope of time is crucial to sharpen our focus and reduce distortion, it is equally important to incorporate, rather than exclude the wisdom of our predecessors. it would thus appear valudie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 32, 1985 able to view language within the context of socialisation (prutting, 1982a) where pragmatics, which is "a pervasive aspect of language which affects the entire communication system" (prutting and kirchner, 1983, p.60) is the core. clinical implications since the measures used in the present study served to highlight different communicative competencies and deficits, clearly one needs to consider the range of pragmatic aspects of language in relation to other aspects of language (foldi et al, 1983; penn, 1983; prutting and kirchner, 1983; prutting et al., 1984; prutting, 1984; wilcox, 1983). more all-encompassing, and dynamic intervention goals could thereby emerge, aimed to enhance structural and functional aspects of communication in relationship to one another. the value of a societal measure such as the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) as a means of approaching the assessment of social competence and a context for interpreting a variety of communicative measures likewise yields several clinical implications. since competence lies in the relational system, the dyad is necessarily the unit of analysis, whose behaviours are judged in terms of societal criteria of appropriateness rather than clinical criteria of correctness. since these criteria take cognisance of the societal values surrounding the client, facilitating a 'real world prognosis' (marshall, 1982 cited by penn, 1983), our criteria for dismissal from remediation will need to shift so that dismissal occurs when the client can manage his/her relationships in a personally appropriate and effective manner within the limitations imposed by his/her stroke. furthermore, intervention goals can emerge directly from the areas assessed by the pragmatic protocol (prutting, 1982b) in which targets are always embedded within this framework (prutting, 1984). such a framework presupposes interactional, contextually based exchanges related to ongoing communicative situations at the level at which the client can meaningfully participate. since the use of this tool serves to highlight the residual compensatory strengths of each individual, rather than simply faulting them for their inaccuracies and linguistic limitations, emerging intervention goals would be based inherently upon a strength, rather than a deficit model. these data and emerging issues constitute a changing paradigm. while the results of the present study have provided some prelimi nary answers, they have raised many more questions. the 1980's will undoubtedly continue to add further in-depth understanding of aphasic pragmatic abilities in relation to other aspects of their communicative competence. in attempting to merge these changing views of theory with practice, aphasiologists need to heed prutting and kirchners' apt comment that "new advances always require a respect for time in order to fit them into our existing schemas (1983, p.48). acknowledgements the writer expresses her sincere gratitude to carol prutting, department of speech and hearing sciences, university of california, santa barbara, for her invaluable guidance as the supervisor of the masters' dissertation on which this paper is based. references davis, g., & wilcox, m.j. incorporating parameters of natural conversation in aphasia treatment. in r. chapey (ed.), language intervention strategies in adult aphasia. baltimore, williams & wilkins, 1981. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 . . „ γ α r h n p r η pragmatic aspects of comfoldi, n.s., cicone μ · > & « . f a g s . l s e g a l o w i t z (ed.), munication in bram-damaged pat ents. ^ y o r ^ l a n g u a g e junctions and brain organisa g o u t s . treasure of ma. new york, w.w. norton and _ £ ° τ 7 μ 1 9 & o'connor, j . k . pragmatic functions in aphasia j. comma, disord., 15, 337-346, 1982. holland, a.l. some practical considerations in aphasia rehabilitation.in m. sullivan and m.s. kommers (eds.), rationale for adult aphasia therapy. nebraska, university of nebraska medical center, 1977. holland, a.l. communicative abilities in daily living. baltimore, university park press, 1980. holland, a.l. observing functional communication of aphasic adults. j. speech hear. disord., 47, 50-56, 1982. holland, a.l. spontaneous recovery from stroke: an investigation of its earliest phases. paper presented at the academy of aphasia, minneapolis, minnesota, 1983. kazdin, a.e. assessing the clinical or applied importance of behaviour change through social validation. behaviour modification, 1, 427-452, 1977. kertesz, a. western aphasia battery. london, ontario canada, university of western ontario, 1980. linebaugh, c.w., kryzer, k.m., oden, s.e. & myers, p.s. reapportionment of communicative burden in aphasia: a study of narrative interactions. in r.h. brookshire (ed.), clinical claire penn aphasiology conference proceedings. minneapolis, mn, brk publishers, 1982. mueller, s.l. an investigation in social competence using clinical and societal profiles. ma thesis, university of california, santa barbara, 1983. penn, m.c. syntactic and pragmatic aspects of aphasic language. doctoral dissertation, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg, south africa, 1983. prutting, c.a. pragmatics as social competence. j. speech hear. disord., al, 123-134, 1982a. prutting, c.a. observational protocol for pragmatic behaviours. developed for the university of california, santa barbara speech and hearing clinic, clinic manual, 1982b. prutting, c.a., & kirchner, d. applied pragmatics. in t. gallagher and c. prutting (eds.), pragmatic assessment and intervention issues in language. san diego, college-hill press, 1983. prutting, c.a., kirchner, d., hassan, p., & buen, p. a societal appraisal of pragmatic behaviours. unpublished manuscript, university of california, santa barbara, 1984. wertz, r.t., weiss, d., kurtzke, j.f., et al., a comparison of clinic, home and deferred treatment of aphasia. veterans administration cooperative study protocol, v.a. hospital, martinez, california, 1978. wertz, r.t. personal communication, v.a. hospital, martinez, california, 1984. wilcox, m.j. aphasia: pragmatic considerations. topics in language disorders, 3, 35-48, 1983. the profile of communicative appropriateness: a clinical tool for the assessment of pragmatics claire penn ph.d (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology & audiology, , university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract i the profile of communicative appropriateness — a newly developed profile for the characterisation of pragmatics is described. the theoretical background to this profile is covered as well as its main components. its application to a group of eighteen aphasic patients is outlined, results suggesting that patient groupings on the profile could be predicted in terms of severity but not in terms of type of aphasia. explanations for this finding are discussed and the potential utility of this profile is suggested. 1 opsomming die profile of communicative appropriateness — 'n nuutontwikkelde profiel vir die karakterisering van pragmatiek word beskryf. die teoretiese rasionaal hieragter en die hoofkomponente van die profiel word behandel. die toepassing hiervan op 'n groep van agtien afatiese pasiente word omskryf resultate dui daarop dat die pasientgroeperings aanduidend kan wees van die erns van afasie maar nie van die tipe afasie nie. verduidelikings hiervoor en die potensiele bruikbaarheid van die profiel word bespreek. the clinical profile is a method of characterising language which has become increasingly popular in recent years. essentially " . . . a linguistic profile is a principled description of . . . those features of a person's . . . use of language which will enable him to be identified for a specific purpose." (crystal, 1982). the format of such a profile is the presentation of a wide range of variables simultaneously so that the clinician is able to see at a glance the communicative assets and deficits of a patient. the main purpose of such a profile according to crystal (1982) is to provide not only a comprehensive description of a patient's data but also an adequate basis for remedial intervention. it is not a standardized measure; nor is it an exhaustive linguistic description. the profile is, however, a compromise to the clinician faced with the realisation that language disability requires comprehensive and individual description. the amount of information contained on a profile is determined by the behaviours being measured © sasha 1985 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 32, 1985 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'ν evaluasie van gedragsterapie by die behandeling van funksioneel disfonie by kinders w. g. nel hoofterapeut by die transvaalse onderwysdepartement, pretoria in hierdie studie is daar gepoog om 'n evaluasie te doen van die gebruik van 'n gedragsterapeutiese benadering by die behandeling van funksionele disfonie by kinders; 'n nuwe benaderingswyse van die probleem is voorgestel ten einde meer permanente resultate te verseker. neurotiese gedrag word deur die gedragsterapeute as aangeleerde gedrag beskou en derhalwe interesseer enige terapie wat gebaseer is op die beginsels van die leerproses, hulle veral.7 die term ,gedragsterapie' is vir die eerste keer deur prof. h. j. eysenck gebruik in 1958 tydens een van sy lesings. die leerbeginsels is alreeds so vroeg as 1927 deur pavlov bestudeer en veral die begrip klassieke kondisionering is deur horn omskryf. tipes kondisionering. vandag is daar twee belangrike tipes kondisionering naamlik: (a) klassieke kondisionering (kortliks, wanneer 'n respons van die proefpersoon onvermydelik is). (b) operant kondisionering (wanneer die eksperimenteerder wag totdat die respons op natuurlike wyse ontstaan). laasgenoemde proses het sy ontstaan van die eksperimentele bevindings van bechterev en thorndike. hulle belangrikste opvolger was b. f. skinner wat dan 00k die term ,operant kondisionering' vir die eerste keer in 1938 gebruik het. kenmerkend van hierdie prosedure, is dat die gedrag van die proefpersoon op sigself instrumenteel is vir die aanbieding of weglating van versterking (beloning) of straf. ν daar is verskeie operante kondisioneringsprosedures wat gebruik kan word, byvoorbeeld gedrag wat 'n aangename verloop vir die individu tot gevolg het sal meer geredelik voorkom as gedrag wat 'n onaangename gevolg vir die individu inhou. indien die terapeut 'n spesifieke responspatroon van die individu wil versterk, behoort opleiding d.m.v. beloning (d.i. wanneer die produksie van 'n spesifieke respons lei tot 'n doelwit waartoe die individu gemotiveer is) die doeltreffendste te wees. opleiding d.m.v. beloning is veral effektief wanneer die gedrag wat die terapeut wil versterk, glad nie aanwesig is nie en ,daar dus weinig is om te beloon. in sulke gevalle begin die terapeut om gedrag wat min of meer lyk op die gewenste gedrag te beloon. wanneer sodanige gedragspatroon voldoende versterk is, kan hoer verwagtinge daargestel word voordat 'n respons beloon word, sodat slegs die gewenste gedrag uiteindelik beloon word, 'n soortgelyke aaneenlopende prosedure word genoem ,shaping' of ,successive approximation'. uit die werk van verskeie navorsers word dit duidelik dat strafopleiding journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. is, no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η evaluasie van gedragsterapie—funksionele disfonie by kinders 15 'n uiters ontoereikende prosedure is om 'n gedragspatroon deurgaans uit te skakel. 'n meer efeektiewe prosedure vir die uitskakeling van 'n sekere gedragspatroon blyk die volgende te wees: nl. om die weglating van ongewenste gedrag aan te moedig deur 'n tydelike uitsluiting van beloning in plaas van spesifieke straf. lg. prosedure is veral van waarde wanneer die mate waarin die ongewenste gedrag voorkom, minder opvallend is. die ongewenste gedrag lei dan tot 'n vermindering van enige positiewe versterking. die pasient kan 00k beloon word in 'n situasie vir die weglating van tot nog toe ongewenste gedrag (weglatingsopleiding), veral wanneer sodanige ongewenste gedrag dikwels voorkom. verskeie skedules vir versterking kan gebruik word. wanneer 'n terapeut 'n sekere gedragspatroon, telkens wanneer dit na vore kom, versterk, toon dit spoedig 'n toename in frekwensie en die terapie bereik sy doelwit. operant kondisionering bied groot geleenthede vir individuele verskille en kan sodoende aangepas word by die pasient sowel as by die terapeut (wat verskillende prosedures kan gebruik). wat die tipe van beloning aanbetref wat die terapeut wil gebruik, is daar verskeie moontlikhede. dit is nodig dat die terapeut die kardinale belang van gekontroleerde versterking aan die pasient besef en derhalwe die verskillende faktore, wat 'n rol mag speel by beloning, voor die aanvang van 'n terapieprogram bepaal. die ondersoek na enige sodanige faktore moet so noukeurig moontlik beplan word, omdat daar nie slegs vasgestel moet word of die gekontroleerde beloning in sy doel slaag nie, maar 00k of daar nie enige ander faktore ongemerk tydens terapie in werking was en verkeerdelik versterk is nie, bv. 'n glimlag van die terapeut kan net soveel beloningswaarde he as 'n ander vorm van beloning. wanneer die terapeut sou glimlag wanneer sy dit nie spesifiek as 'n beloning op 'n sekere'moment wil gebruik nie, sou die operante kondisioneringsprosedure nie konsekwent deurgevoer word nie. hieruit blyk dit duidelik dat die rol van die terapeut tydens 'n operante kondisioneringsprosedure nie maklik is nie. dit is verder noodsaaklik dat die terapiesessies noukeurig geadministreer word: dit is van die uiterste belang by die evaluasie van die terapie en kan dien as 'n waardevolle hulpmiddel vir die terapeut om haar eie doelstellings te hersien en om haar eie gedrag, waar moontlik, reg te stel. operante kondisioneringsterapie. wanneer operante kondisioneringsterapie begin word, is dit veral van belang dat die onmiddellike omgewing van die pasient deeglik op hoogte is van die terapieprogram en 'n goeie insig daarin het. die nodige instruksies moet dus vooraf gegee word aan die ouers, onderwysers, verpleegsters ens. of wie 00k al tydens die terapie met die kind gemoeid is, sodat die pasient nie buite die terapiesessies met heeltemal 'n verskillende beloningsisteem gekonfronteer word nie.1 by die behandeling van die disfoniese kind moet die ouers veral saamwerk; van die uiterste belang is om toe te sien dat die ouers nie die gebruik van die verkeerde stemgewoonte versterk deur aandag daaraan te gee juis wanneer dit voorkom nie. straf het skynbaar geen motiverende waarde vir die kind om die goeie stemtoon te gebruik nie (soos wat tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. is, nr. 1: des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) w. g. nel j6 negatiewe oefening het vir die vaslegging van 'n sekere klank in spontane spraak nie): dit plaas alleen groter druk op die kind. die ouer kan die terapiesessies bywoon ten einde te leer om die goeie stemkwaliteit onmiddellik te herken (sodat dit tuis ook beloon kan word). die moeder kan van die uiterste hulp wees om situasies waar die stem tuis minder goed was, aan die terapeut te rapporteer. vol gens rachman6 kan die toepassing van die operante kondisioneringstegnieke, sowel as meer bekende metodes'van gedragsterapie, verder van onskatbare waarde wees, veral in die gebied van die kindersielkunde. die algemene oorsaak van kinderdisfoniee, is stembandknobbeltjies, wat te wyte kan wees aan oorsake soos 'n verkeerde spreekstemtoonhoogte, ooreising van die stem deur oormatige geskreeu of 'n te harde insit van die aanvangsklanke ens.8 volgens greene is die heropleiding van kinderstemme moeilik, omdat permanente resultate a.g.v. hulle ouderdomme, nie rnaklik verkry word deur stemoefeninge, of deur hulle te belet om hulle stemme te ooreis nie. spraakterapie behoort dus eers op elfjange leeftyd in aanvang te neem indien die disfonie dan nog bestaan. sy is van mening dat die herstel van die toestand op hierdie ouderdom ook spontaan kan geskied. in alle gevalle van stemmisbruik het die toestand van die pasient se vokale patroon heelwat te doen met sy aangebore konstitusie, sy algemene gesondheid, sy vatbaarheid vir verkoues en sy reaksie op spanning. babas wat hees word na baie huil, mag later in hulle lewens as volwassenes disfoniee ontwikkel.4 damste noem ook die probleem van hoe 'n permanente verandering in 'n foutiewe stemgewoonte verkry kan word.2 heelwat van die kinders met stemprobleme wat onlangs by die afdeling spraakpatologie van die universiteitskliniek te utrecht ondersoek is, toon duidelike tekens van gespannenheid en kom senuweeagtig van geaardheid voor. indien hierdie senuweeagtigheid uitgeskakel kon word (afgeleer kon word volgens die gedragsterapeute deur die proses van wederkerige remming van angs) sou die verkryging van meer permanente resultate makliker wees. die belangrikste aanduiding vir die noodsaaklikheid van verdere navorsing in die moontlike toepassing van tegnieke soos hierbo beskryf in die gebied van kinderdisfoniee, is 'n verslag deur gray, england en mahoney3 oor die suksesvolle behandeling van 'n volwassene met stembandknobbeltjies, deur middel van gedragsterapie volgens die metode van wederkerige remming soos beskryf deur wolpe.9 die tegniek van ,sistematiese ongevoeligmaak' wat gebruik'word by die proses van wederkerige remming, blyk uiters bruikbaar te wees vir die stemterapeut ten einde meer permanente resultate te verkry by die behandeling van funksionele disfoniee. dit kan veral van hulp wees by die oordrag van die nuwe stem na situasies wat nog vir die pasient angs mag inhou. volgens wolpe is dit nie noojdsaaklik om die gevreesde voorwerp of situasie werklik aan te bied nie, aangesien daar slegs van 'n angshierargie gebruik gemaak kan word. sodoende.kan hierdie metode van werk veral tydbesparend wees. journal of the'south'african logopedic society, vol. 15, no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η evaluasie van gedragsterapie—funksionele disfonie by kinders opsomming van bevindings 17 stemoefeninge < ten einde goeie stemtoon te verkry. beginsels soos o.k. kan gebruik word om kind te motiveer. effektiewe beloning dan noodsaaklik. w.r. vir oordrag van „nuwe" stem na situasies waarin kind nog nie bereid is om sy „goeie" te gebruik nie. heesheid < senuwee-agtigheidgeen direkte gedragsterapie vir vermindering van angs nie; geen kind het 'n fobie gehad nie; sou van waarde wees vir die psigotiese kind. o.k. = operant kondisionering. w.r. = wederkerige remming. >verander omgewing behandel ouers volgens gedragsterapeutiese tegnieke alleen as kind enige spesifieke gedragsprobleme openbaar het; geen een van die ouers het die kind vir sodanige probleem gebring operante kondisioneringsprosedures—met die gebruik van effektiewe versterking—kan dus van nut wees ten einde meer permanente resultate te verkry en om die kind sover te kry om die goeie stem so gou moontlik te gebruik (stemoefeninge kan van nut wees om 'n beter stemtoon o.l.v. die stemterapeut te verkry; die kind moet leer om die goeie en die minder goeie stemtoon te onderskei). daar kon slegs 'n begin gemaak word met die behandeling van kinders en derhalwe is die deel van die ondersoek as 'n voorstudie beskou. alhoewel hierdie werkmetode veel van die terapeut verg en oenskynlik tydrowend is, is dit 'n intensiewer werkswyse, gegrond op 'n meer wetenskaplike beskouing van die verskillende beloningsisteme as sodanig. afgesien van die waarde van effektiewe beloning waarop dit wys, dui dit 00k op die noodsaaklikheid van verdere gekontroleerde eksperimentele toepassing van gedragsterapie op die gebied van funksionele stemstoornisse. hierdie studie is gedoen onder leiding van dr. p. h. damste, hoof van die afdeling foniatrie, aan die universiteitskliniek te utrecht. opsomming terapie gebaseer op beginsels van die leerteorie word bespreek, en die behavioristiese benadering tot terapie vir kinders met knobbels op die stemplooie word ge-evalueer. summary therapy based on learning principles is discussed, and a behaviourist approach to the difficulties of therapy for children with nodular laryngitis is evaluated. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 75, nr. 1: des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 w: g. net bibliografie i bakker, b. (1967): operante konditionering—ongepubliseerde lesing in kinderpsigiatrie gehou te utrecht op 9 november 1967 by die• ,snchting academisch ziekenhuis utrecht.' . „ 2. damste, p. h. (1967): stembandknobbeltjies. logopedie en foniatrie, 39, 95-9». j. b. wolters, groningen. , t 7 3. gray, β. b., england, g., and mahoney, j. l. (1965): treatment of vocal nodules by reciprocal inhibition. journal of behaviour research and therapy, 3, 187-193, london. , , _ . · , 4. greene, m. c. l. (1957): the voice and its disorders. pitman medical publishing co., ltd. τ τ 5. mednick, s. a. (1964): psychologie van het leren. prentice hall, inc., englewood cliffs, n.j. 6. rachman, s. (1962): child psychology and learning theory. journal ot child psychology and psychiatry, 2, 149-163. 7. santer-weststrate, h. c. (1964): gedragsterapie. van gorcum and co. 8. wilson, d. k. (1961): children with vocal nodules. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 26, 19-26. . 9. wolpe, j. (1958): psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. stanford university press, stanford, california, and witwatersrand university press, johannesburg. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 15, no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'ν kommunikatiefgefundeerde ondersoek να bepaalde waarnemingsverskynsels by disfemie r e n e h u g o m.a. (log.) (pretoria) spraak-, stem en gehoorkliniek, universiteit van pretoria ops om μ ing hierdie studieprojek stel h o m t e n d o e l die bestudering van bepaalde o u d i t i e w e waarnemingsvermoens b y disfemie, s o o s gesien vanuit 'n k o m m u m k a t i e w e o o g p u n t . eksperiment 1: stel h o m ten doel die bepaling van suiwertoonsensitiwiteit en spraakdiskriminasievermoens van 'n groep van 10 hakkelaars, en die vergelyking van hierdie gegewens met 'n kontrole groep nie-hakkelaars. die resultaat dui o p 'n betekenisvolle verskil,tussen die suiwertoondrempelwaardes van die regterore van die hakkelaars en nie-hakkelaars en kan m o o n t l i k in verband gebring word met s o w e l terugvoeringsafwykings as gehoordominansieteoriee. eksperiment 2: b e o o g die bepaling van waarnemingsdrempels van neutrale en negatiewe w o o r d e (woorde waarop gewoonlik gehakkel word) b y 'n groep hakkelaars, en die vergelyking hiervan met 'n aantal nie-hakkelaars. die resultate t o o n 'n betekenisvolle verskil tussen hierdie 2 lyste vir die hakkelaars, maar nie vir die vlotsprekers nie. ter verklaring kan aangevoer word dat die hakkelaars, o p grond van e m o s i o n e l e kwaliteite verbonde aan die negatiewe w o o r d e , hierdie w o o r d e makliker waarneem as die vlotspreker, waarskynlik deur gebruik te maak van perseptuele verdedigingsmeganismes soos perseptuele ingestemdheid. eksperiment 3 : is gerig op die bepaling van die bestaan van onwaarneembaarheid van betekenisvolle stimuli b y die hakkelaar t y d e n s die h a k k e l p e n o d e . geen sodanige verskynsel kan betekenisvol a a n g e t o o n word nie, h o e w e l daar in die bespreking daarop g e w y s is dat 'n verandering in die eksperimentele prosedures nogtans so 'n f e n o m e e n kan u i t w y s . in sy geheel gesien, is daar dus in hierdie studieprojek bepaalde waarnemingsabnormaliteite b y die disfemie a a n g e t o o n wat nie b y die vlotspreker gevind kan w o r d nie. hierdie abnormaliteite dui essensieel op 'n wyer siemng van disfemie nl., as 'n komplekse, k o m m u n i k a t i e w e probleem. summary in this s t u d y certain aspects of auditory perception in cases of dysphemia are studied, emphasis being o n c o m m u n i c a t i o n aspects. experiment 1: has as its object the determination of pure-tone sensitivity and speech discrimination abilities of a group of 1 0 stutterers, and t h e comparison of these data with a control group of non-stutterers. the results point to a sienificant difference b e t w e e n the pure-tone threshold values of the right ears of the stutterers and the non-stutterers, and may b e linked with feedback deficiences and with theories on auditory d o m i n a n c e . tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 19. desember 197 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 0 rene hugo experiment 2: sets o u t to determine perceptual thresholds of neutral and negative words (words which are usually stuttered on) in the case of a group of stutterers, and to compare these threshold values with t h o s e of non-stutterers. the results s h o w a significant difference b e t w e e n these t w o lists for the stutterers, b u t not for fluent speakers. a suggested explanation is that the stutterers perceive negative words more readily than fluent speakers d o , because of the e m o t i o n a l qualities c o n n e c t e d with these words, probably b y putting up perceptual defence mechanisms such as the perceptual set. experiment 3 : aims, at the determination of the e x i s t e n c e of imperceptivity with regard t o meaningful stimuli in the case of the stutterer during the stuttering b l o c k . n o such p h e n o m e n o n can b e proven, although in the discussion it is indicated that a variation of the experimental procedure m a y point to such a p h e n o m e n o n . in general this s t u d y thus presents certain perceptual abnormalities evidenced in dysphemia cases, which are n o t found in fluent speakers. these abnormalities clearly p o i n t t o the need for a broader c o n c e p t of dysphemia. it must b e seen as a c o m p l e x problem. edward sapir9 het te kenne gegee: „human beings do not live in the objective world alone, nor alone in the world of social activity as ordinarily understood, but are very much at the mercy of the particular language which has become the medium of expression for their society". hierdie afhanklikheid word groter en verkry 'n definitiewe negatiewe kleur, sodra die taalvermoe bepaalde nie-normale verskynsels vertoon. die verskynsel van disfemie kan waarskynlik as een van die grootste en mees omvattende abnormaliteite beskou word, omdat dit die kommunikasievermoe as geheel betrek.3 daarom is dit aanvaarbaar om te verwag dat die motoriese herhalingsen verlengingsabnormaliteite ook 'n sensoriese (perseptuele?) komponent moet he. selfs al word disfemie in die lig van 'n leerproses, eerder as 'n fisiese of neurale afwyking gesien, moet dit nogtans 'n sensoriese komponent besit as in ag geneem word dat mowrer verklaar het: „ all learning takes place on the sensory side".3 daar bestaan dan op die huidige stadium heelwat teoriee en bespiegelings wat hierdie gebied betrek. in 'n poging om al die uiteenlopende benaderings tot persepsieafwykings en disfemie as 'n eenheid te sien, kan die benadering van freund as verteenwoordigend dien: / persepsie-afwykings by die hakkelaar as spreker. in die verhouding tussen innerlike spraak en uiterlike hakkel, kan gevind word dat die bespeuring van 'n ,moeilike' woord op 'n baie vroee stadium van sinsamestelling plaasvind. op 'n stadium wat die woord self onhoorbaar en onsigbaar is vir die innerlike oor, d.w.s., op 'n preverbale, preperseptuele stadium, kan die moontlike aanwesigheid van die woord alreeds deur leidrade van ongemak ervaar word. op hierdie wyse kan modifikasie, in 'n poging tot vermyding, alreeds op 'n onbewuste vlak plaasvind. dus: „even in this 'deep' perceptual layer of inner speech, the complex of disturbances exerts its influence". journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ' kommunikatiefgefundeerde ondersoek 41 persepsie-afwykings by die hakkelaar as waarnemer. die oomblik van hakkel gaan gepaard met 'n groot aantal perseptuele distorsies, nl.: i. verkleining van die bewussynsveld; ii. werklike perseptuele distorsies. ondersoekers soos johnson en william het hierdie abnormaliteite as die natuurlike gevolg van die hakkelaar se foutiewe self-evaluasie verklaar. freund beweer egter dat die foutiewe evaluasies sekonder, en nie aanleidend is, tot die perseptuele distorsies nie. gedurende die hoogtepuiit van hakkel is daar 'n gerigte affektief-outosuggestiewe aandagkonsentrering op die struikelblok. hierdie konsentrasie is van so 'n intensiewe aard dat die gepaardgaande nodige perseptuele aksies verlore gaan. freund noem hierdie verskynsel „kinesthetic illusion" en verklaar dit as die gevolg van 'n hoogs emosionele toestand waartydens 'n persoon sy hulpeloosheid ervaar in die aanwesigheid van onoorkomelike probleme. hierdie terme is soortgelyk aan die deur hans w. meur gebruik en deur froeschels oorgeneem nl., „catathymia" wat die invloed van sterk affektief-belaaide kompleksiteite op bewustheid en persepsie impliseer.4 metode dit word gehipoteseer dat die hakkelaar bepaalde ouditiewe waarnemingsabnormaliteite vertoon wat: a) voorkom tydens die hakkelperiode; b) die waarnemingsdrempel sal beinvloed. hierdie verskynsels sal waarskynlik nie aanwesig wees by die nie-hakkelaar nie. daar sal gepoog word om aan te toon dat hierdie waarnemingsabnormaliteite, indien dit wel voorkom, nie so afwykend is as wat aanvanklik gemeen is nie. dit hou eerder verband met „normale" verdedigingsmeganisme — in soverre as wat verdedigingsmeganisme as normaal beskryf kan word — teen dreigende stimuli. probleemstelling dit is van 'n drievoudige aard nl.: 1. is die suiwertoonkurwe en spraakdiskriminasievermoens van die hakkelaar vergelykbaar met die van die nie-hakkelaar? indien nie, op waiter gebiede sal hierdie verskille voorkom? 2. is daar 'n verskil tussen die waarnemingsdrempel van neutrale en emosioneeldreigende woorde by die hakkelaar? kom hierdie verskil, indien dieselfde woorde gebruik word, ook by die nie-hakkelaar voor? 3. toon die hakkelaar bepaalde onwaarneembaarheidskenmerke tydens die hakkelperiode? indien wel, kan dit in.verband gebring word met die aard van die stimuli? tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 2 rene hugo apparaat die maico m.a. 24 dubbelkanaal-oudiometer (standaard i.s.o.) gelee in 'n klankdigte tweekamer-kompleks kamer, is gebruik. proefpersone 1. eksperimentele groep bestaan uit 'n groep van 10 hakkelaars tussen die ouderdomme 16-31 jaar. die groep is verkry uit verwysings van die spraak-, stemen gehoorkliniek, universiteit van pretoria. omdat die eksperimente 'n redelike mate van insig in die probleem vereis, is slegs die hakkelaars geselekteer wat 'n deelname in terapie het en wat deur hul terapeut as volwasse eedefinieer is. 2. kontrolegroep — bestaan uit 10 nie-hakkelaars, ook tussen die ouderdom van 16-31 jaar. hierdie groep is geselekteer met die doel om te verseker dat hulle homogeen in alle opsigte is, in vergelyking met die eksperimentele groep, behalwe vir die element van vlotheidsversteuring. om dit te verseker, is gebruik gemaak van die metode van afgepaarde vergelykings waar die volgende faktore konstant gehou is: ouderdom, geslag, intellektuele peil, sosio-ekonomiese klas en swangerskappe. eksperimente drie verskillende eksperimente word uitgevoer, en in hierdie verslag sal die eksperimente apart behandel word. eksperiment i doel: die bepaling van suiwertoonsensitiwiteit en spraakdiskriminasievermoens. materiaal: i. vir die suiwertoontoets is gebruik: 'n suiwertoonoudiogram wat frekwensies 125, 250, 1 000, 2 000,4 000, 8 000 hz asook die intensiteitsvlak 0-110 db-gehoorpeil (iso. 1964) aandui. hierdie oudiogram is vir elke oor afsonderlik gemeet en laat toe vir optekening van luggeleidings-, beengeleidingsen smalbandmaskeringswaarde. ii. vir die spraakdiskriminasietelling: 6 fonetiesgebalanseerde woordelyste van 25 woorde elk opgestel deur die wnnr (1962) is gebruik. daar is gepoog om hoofsaaklik een waarde te bepaal nl., 100% korr.ekte spraakdiskriminasie. 1 / metode: i. soos gebruik vir die suiwertoontoets. die aanbieding is by 1 000 hz begin, gevolg deur die lae frekwensies, weer 1 000hz om die eerste bepaling te bevestig en laastens die hoe frekwensies. die intensiteitsvermeerdering het in diskrete stappe van 5 db plaasgevind. die drempel (def: die intensiteitsvlak waar die proefpersoon 50% reageer op die stimulusaanbiedinge) is bepaal deur 'n kombinasie van die stygende en dalende metode. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ',n kommunikatiefgefundeerde ondersoek 4 3 ii. soos gebruik vir die spraakdiskriminasietelling. die aanbieding is begin by 'n intensiteitswaarde ongeveer 20-30 db bokant die gemiddelde suiwertoonkurwe. verdere verhogings/verlaging het berus op die reaksie van die proefpersoon totdat die persentasiewaardes 50-100% gedek is. resultaat (ij die resultate word in tabel i uitgele. proef persone • geslag regteroor linheroor ε 1 μ 6 , 6 7 5 , 0 0 §· ε 2 μ 1 1 , 6 7 1 1 , 6 7 fe e 3 v 8 , 3 3 1 0 , 0 0 n> ε 4 μ 1 3 , 3 3 7 3 , 3 3 i ε 5 μ 2 3 , 3 3 1 0 , 0 0 ε e 6 "ε ε 7 μ 0 , 0 0 1,67 ε e 6 "ε ε 7 μ 6 , 6 7 5 , 0 0 i · ε 8 v 1 1 , 6 7 1 0 , 0 0 3 ε 9 v 6 , 6 7 5 , 0 0 ε 1 0 μ 1 3 , 3 3 1 5 , 0 0 κ 1 1 0 , 0 0 11,67 κ 2 6,67 8,33 •α κ 3 6,67 5,00 ο κ 4 3,33 5,00 » κ 5 8,33 5,00 ε κ β 5,00 11,67 i κ 7 3,33 1,67 ϋ< κ 8 3,33 1,67 κ 9 5,00 5,00 κ10 8,33 5,00 tab el i gemiddelde suiwertoon luggeleidingsdrempel in ab van frekwensies 512, 1024, 2048 hz. ter verklaring van hierdie gegewens is dan gepoog om aandag aan die volgende resultaatverwerkings te gee: 1. 'n vergelyking tussen die gemiddelde suiwertoondrempelwaardes van die linkeren regterore van die eksperimentele groep, waartydens die nulhipotese, dat daar geen verskil tussen hierdie ore bestaan nie, gestel is. geen betekenisvolle verskil kan, wat betref die suiwertoondrempelwaardes, tussen die linkeren regterore van die eksperimentele groep aangetoon word deur toepassing van die tekentoets nie. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 4 rene hugo 2. 'η vergelyking tussen die gemiddelde suiwertoondrempelwaardes van die regterore van die eksperimentele en kontrolegroepe. met behulp van die mann-whitney-wilcoxon-toets kan aangetoon word dat die suiwertoondrempelwaardes van die regterore van die eksperimentele groep verskil, op die 2%-peil van betekenis, van die van die kontrolegroep. 3. 'n vergelyking tussen die gemiddelde suiwertoondrempelwaardes van die linkerore van die eksperimeritele en kontrolegroepe. in hierdie geval egter kan geen beduidende verskil d.m.v. die mann-whitney-wilcoxon-toets aangetoon word nie. 4. 'n vergelyking tussen die gemiddelde suiwertoondrempelwaardes van mans en vrouens van die eksperimentele groep. die gebruik van statistiese verwerkings is in hierdie geval nie wenslik nie, hoofsaaklik as gevolg van die te klein aantal proefpersone wat tot ongemagtigde gevolgtrekkings kan lei. wat hierdie aspek betref kan daar dus volstaan word met verwysing na die feit dat daar korresponderende drempelwaardes vir die vroulike lede van die eksperimentele groep, die mans van die eksperimentele groep en ook die .kontrolerende groep, bestaan. a f l e i d i n g s (i) samevattend wil dit dus voorkom asof daar wel 'n verskil tussen die suiwertoonsensitiwiteit van die hakkelaar en die nie-hakkelaar bestaan. uit hierdie eksperiment word die verskil slegs vir die regterore aangetoon 'n feit wat moontlik betekenisvol kan saamhang met ander toepaslike faktore soos liggaamsvoorkeur en hemisfeerdominansie. voordat dit egter volkome aanvaar kan word, moet in aanmerking geneem word dat die eerste verwerking nl., 'n vergelyking tussen die regteren linkerore van die eksperimentele groep, geen betekenisvolle verskille aangetoon het nie. r e s u l t a a t (ii) met betrekking tot die tweede deel van eksperiment 1 nl., die bepaling van spraakdiskriminasievermoens, is veral op twee waardes, 100% en 50% gelet. dit is aangevul met 'n verskil tussen die twee persentasiewaardes in 'n poging om die verloop van die spraakkurwe numeries voor te stel. die resultate word in tabel ii uitgele. a f l e i d i n g s (ii) ; y hoewel statistiese verwerkings in hierdie geval moeilik gedoen kon word a:g.v. die voorkoms van 'n groot aantal gelyke waardes, wil dit tog oor die algemeen voorkom asof daar nie 'n betekenisvolle verskil tussen die verskillende gegewens aangetoon kan word nie. i eksperiment ii 1 doel: die bepaling van die waarnemingsdrempel van bepaalde neutrale en journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ' kommunikatiefgefundeerde ondersoek 45 proeffa c ύί 0 intensiteitspeil in desibel proeffa c ύί 0 geslag regteroor linkeroor jsttrsu ftc verskil jsttrsu ftc 100% 50% verskil 100% 50% verskil ε 1 μ 3 0 15 15 3 0 15 15 •ci. s ε 2 μ 4 0 20 2 0 3 0 15 15 sj ε 3 v 3 0 15 15 3 0 15 15 i ε 4 μ 4 0 25 15 — — | ε 5 μ 5 0 35 15 4 0 2 0 2 0 j ε 6 μ 3 0 15 15 20 15 5 ϊ ε 7 μ 3 0 2 0 10 3 0 15 15 t ε 8 v 3 0 15 15 3 0 15 15 ^ ε 9 v 3 0 10 2 0 2 0 15 5 ε 1 0 > μ 3 0 15 15 3 0 10 20 κ 1 4 0 25 15 4 0 20 2 0 κ 2 3 0 20 10 3 0 15 15 « . κ 3 2 0 25 5 3 0 15 15 i κ 4 4 0 25 15 3 0 15 15 ι" κ 5 3 0 10 2 0 3 0 15 15 ε κ 6 3 0 10 2 0 30 15 15 1 κ 7 2 0 10 10 3 0 10 2 0 ^ κ 8 3 0 10 2 0 3 0 10 2 0 κ 9 3 0 10 20 2 0 10 10 κ10 30 10 20 3 0 15 15 tabel ii spraakdiskriminasiewaardes. vermoedelike negatiewe stimuli, aangebied in die vorm van spesifieke woordelyste. materiaal: i. neutrale woorde: woorde wat as sodanig ge-evalueer is deur 'n groep proefpersone nadat dit d.m.v. 'n tachistoskoop aan hulle gebied is. ii. hakkelwoorde — woorde wat vir elke hakkelaar spesifieke ,jiakkelwaarde" besit as gevolg van die voortdurende hakkelervaring, wat met negatiewe emosies geassosieer is. bogenoemde 2 groepe het aan bepaalde vereistes voldoen, nl.: a. eenlettergrepig omdat drempelbepaling sodoende meer definitief is. b. die woorde moes onder die 1 000 mees algemene afrikaanse woorde voorkom (vereiste gestel deur broadbent). tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir sprak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 6 rene hugo metode 1. aan die proefpersone van die eksperimentele groep is 'n lys van 12 neutrale woorde (soos voorheen bepaal) gebied waaruit hulle alle potensiele hakkelwoorde moes elimineer. indien nodig is 'n verdere aantal woorde deur die proefleier op 'n doelbewus-toevallige wyse uitgeskakel sodat 6 woorde oorgebly het wat.as die neutrale stimulilys sou dien. 2. 'n lys van 6 hakkelwoorde is saamgestel op grond van ondervraging en observasie van die proefpersone. 3. 'n finale stimulus-lys is saamgestel, bestaande uit 6 hakkelen 6 neutrale woorde. die orderangskikking het berus op 'n toevallige metode deur die gebruik van ewekansige tabelle. 4. die binourale drempelwaarde van elke woord is vervolgens bepaal d.m.v. die stygende metode wat in deskrete 5 db-stappe aangebied is. 5. die stimulus-lys van die neutrale woorde asook 'n saamgestelde lys van die totale aantal hakkelwoorde soos verkry van al 10 die proefpersone van die eksperimentele groep, is ook aan die kontrolegroep gebied en hulle drempelwaardes bepaal. r e s u l t a a t die resultate word in tabel iii uitgele. +proefpersone g. eksperimentelecroep kontrolegroep proefpersone +proefpersone g. neutrale woorde hakkelwoorde hakkelwoorde neutrale woorde proefpersone ε 1 μ 18,00 17,50 24,46 11,25 κ 1 ε 2 μ 20,00 17,00 17,77 13,64 κ 2 ε 3 v 20,00 15,00 11,40 14,09 κ 3 ε 4 μ 33,00 29,00 18,86 21,50 κ 4 ε 5 μ 23,00 21,25 14,00 20,45 κ 5 ε 6 μ 17,00 14,17 18,46 22,14 κ 6 ε 7 μ 21,67 22,00 17,75 21,00 κ 7 ε 8 v 21,67 17,00 13,82 14,58 κ 8 ε 9 v 15,83 15,00 8,94 10,00 κ 9 ε10 μ 15,00 12,00 14,00 14,09 κ10 tabel ιπ drempelwaardes in db verwerking van hierdie gegewens is hoofsaaklik op twee vlakke gedoen nl.; 1. 'n vergelyking tussen die eksperimentele groep se waarnemingsdrempels vir neutrale en hakkelwoorde waartydens die nulhipotese verklaar dat daar geen verskil bestaan nie. die resultate van die wilcoxon-simmetrietoets toon journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ' kommunikatiefgefundeerde ondersoek 4 7 wel 'η verskil op die 1%-peil van betekenis en verwerp dus die nulhipotese. dit wil se, die eksperimentele groep vertoon 'n hoogs betekenisvolle verskil in drempelwaardes tussen die 2 lyste en wel in di6 sin dat die waarnemingsdrempels van die negatiewe (hakkel) woorde betekenisvol laer is. 2. 'n vergelyking tussen die kontrolegroep as waarnemingsdrempels vir neutrale en hakkelwoorde. weereens υρ grond van die resultate verkry deur die wilcoxon-simmetrietoets moet in hierdie geval verklaar word dat die nulhipotese nie verwerp kan word nie, d.i., daar kan nie 'n betekenisvolle verskil tussen die drempelwaardes vir negatiewe (hakkel) en neutrale woorde gevind word nie. gesien die betekenisvolle resultate van eksperiment 1 is enige verdere kruisvergelyking (bv., vergelyking tussen eksperimentele en kontrolegroep se hakkelwoorddrempelwaardes) moeilik uitvoerbaar en sal dit waarskynlik geen praktiese bydrae lewer nie. dieselfde besware vir 'n vergelyking tussen die resultate van manlike en vroulike lede wat op eksperiment 1 ingebring is, geld ook hier en is dus nie deurgevoer nie. wat die kwalitatiewe resultate betref, kan dit betekenisvol wees om op die volgende te let: a) die lede van die eksperimentele groep wat die grootste verskil in drempelwaarde vertoon nl., e3, e4, e8, is diegene wat, volgens die proefleier se mening (let wel: dit is nie statistics d.m.v. vergelykingsskale vasgestel nie) die ernstigste ritmiese versteurings vertoon. in hierdie geval word met .ernstig' die frekwensie sowel as die graad van spanning tydens die onvlothede bedoel. b) in teenstelling hiermee is die proefpersoon, e7, wat 'n negatiewe verskil tussen sy drempelwaardes vertoon, 'n hakkelaar wat deur geringe uiterlike spanning en min vlotheidsprobleme gekenmerk word. c) proefpersone e3 en e8 wat 2 uit die 3 lede is wat 'n groot verskil in drempelwaarde vertoon, is vroulike lede van die groep. a f l e i d i n g s om tot wilde bewerings in verband met 'n definitiewe etiologie te kom op grond van hierdie resultate sou buitensporig wees. nogtans wil dit voorkom asof die gegewens op 'n aantal aspekte lig kan werp: 1. die feit dat die eksperimentele groep 'n betekenisvolle verskil in drempelwaarde tussen die negatiewe en hakkelwoorde vertoon, lei tot vrae in verband met die oorsaak hiervan. aangesien in ag geneem is dat die 2 woordelyste homogeen is wat betref aspekte soos lengte (eenlettergrepig), bekendheid (1 000 mees bekende woorde), en bewustheid (albei lyste is aan die proefpersoon gebied voor die eksperiment) wil dit voorkom asof die oorsaak van die verskil by 'n ander faktor gesoek moet word. die outeur reken dat bekendheidskwaliteite moontlik nogtans 'n rol kan speel as in aanmerking geneem word dat die definiering van „hakkelwoord" soos volg daar uitsien: „woorde waarop jy gewoonlik of altyd sal hakkel". dit kan dus veroorsaak dat hierdie woorde, al is hul algemene gebruiksfrekwensie net. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 19 desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 8 rene hugo so hoog soos die neutrale woorde, tog vir die indiwiduele hakkelaar meer bekend sal wees. nogtans is hierdie verklaring nie voldoende nie en moet die bekendheidsfaktor ook met die emosionele kwaliteit saamhang. dit is dus meer waarskynlik dat die verskil in drempelwaardes toegeskryf moet word aan die feit dat dit woorde is waarop gewoonlik gehakkel word, wat dus altyd gepaard gaan met sterk emosionele negatiewe ervaririgs en wat dus die waarnemingsdrempel op een of ander wyse sal beinvloed. gesien in die lig van die strydvraag omtrent die bestaan, al dan nie, van drempelwaardes is dit op hierdie stadium gewaagd om die resultate van hierdie eksperiment toe te skryf aan die verskynsel van subliminale persepsie en perseptuele sensitiwiteit (def.: die waarnemer reageer diskriminerend op die emosionele aspekte van 'n stimulus wat subliminaal aangebied word en wel in die sin dat emosioneeldreigende stimuli makliker waargeneem word.1 >6) nogtans is dit moontlik en wenslik om dit vergelykbaar te stel aan 'n verdedigingsmeganisme sg., aan perseptuele ingestemdheid. dit kan dan gedefinieer word as: 'n hoogs algemene toestand van gereedheid in die waarnemer om selektief te reageer op klasse van gebeure in sy omgewing. dit sou interessant wees om te sien op waiter wyse hierdie bevindings in verband gebring kan word met faktore soos: erns van die probleem, prognose, persoonlikheid en algemene lewensaanpasbaarheid van die indiwidu. 2. bepaalde disfemie-kenmerke kan met hierdie resultate in verband gebring word bv.: a) die hakkelaar se vermoe om moeilike woorde en selfs situasies vooraf „aan te voel" op 'n stadium wat dit nog nie werklik sy bewussynsveld betrek het nie. b) aan die anderkant kan dit ook verband hou met die hakkelaar se skynbare onbewustheid van sekondere simptome, en selfs met die verskynsel van onwaarneembaarheid.5 die energiekonsentrasie en waarnemingsensitiwiteit word gerig op die gevreesde woord, aangesien dit vir hom die essensie van sy probleem is en kan lei tot 'n afname in die waarnemingsvermoens op ander vlakke. 3. hierdie resultate kan ook ten nouste in verband gebring word met terapieresultate. op hierdie stadium sal alle spraakterapeute wat in die praktyk staan onomwonde verklaar dat die grootste struikelblok vir habilitasie van die disfemie, die korrekte gebruik van voorbereidende instellings tydens sosiale situasies is. waar 'n verdedigingsmeganisme (in hierdie geval die „vooraf ingestemdheid jeens die woord") as 'n wyse van aanpassing aan die gemeenskap is, kan dit miskien as 'n onmoontlikheid beskou word om van die hakkelaar te verwag om die resultaat van hierdie aanpassing te modifiseer ten einde beheer oor sy spraak te verkry. aan die ander kant kan dit dus ook as 'n prognostiese teken met betrekking tot die mate waartoe beheertegnieke wel sal slaag, beskou word. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ' kommunikatiefgefundeerde ondersoek 4 9 eksperiment iii doel: die vasstelling van die moontlike bestaan van onwaarneembaarheid („imperceptivity" — volgens froeschels: „shut in to such a degree that they did not perceive certain signals which they perceive normally..."5) van betekenisvolle stimuli by die hakkelaar tydens 'n hakkelperiode. materiaal: potlood en blankopapier. dieselfde woordelyste soos gebruik in eksperiment ii wat bestaan uit 6 neutrale en 6 hakkelwoorde met 'n verandering in die volgorde van aanbieding. metode 1. die proefpersone van die eksperimentele groep is gekeur op grond van aard van hulle spesifieke vlotheidsversteurings. slegs die persone is gebruik wat so 'n lang hakkelperiode as simptoom vertoon dat hulle ten tye daarvan suksesvol met 'n woord gestimuleer kan word. eventueel is 5 proefpersone gebruik. 2. gegrond op die gegewens van eksperiment i is al die proefpersone se mees gemaklike luidheid, binouraal, vir spraakwaarneming bepaal. 3. die proefpersone is opgedra om oor te gaan tot 'n spontane lang beskrywingsreaksie (bv. „beskryf die pad wat u moet ry vanaf u huis tot by die spraakkliniek"). hiertydens is dan van die proefpersone verwag om te luister vir die voorkoms van bepaalde stimuluswoorde, om indien hulle dit hoor, te reageer deur die woord neer te skryf, en om dit verder te ignoreer en aan te gaan met die „gesprek". 4. tydens die beskrywingsituasie is die stimuluswoord aangebied sodra 'n hakkelperiode voorgekom het en net nadat dit begin is. vir hierdie doel is die hakkelperiode gedefinieer as „ what the onlooker observes is the so-called stuttering block . . . repetition, prolongation or a complete cessation of sound".3 5. bogenoemde resultate is kwalitatief aangevul d.m.v. gesprekke en vraelyste waartydens gepoog is om vas te stel of waarnemingsprobleme tydens die hakkelperiode ondervind is. r e s u l t a a t die resultate word in tabel iv uitgele. gesien die klein getal proefpersone is dit in hierdie geval onwenslik gereken om die gegewens statisties te verwerk. oor die algemeen kan die volgende gevolgtrekkings gemaak word: i. uit die 5 proefpersone het al 5 tenminste een defektiewe waarneming gemaak (met defektief word sowel verkeerde as geen waarneming bedoel); tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 5 0 rene hugo proefpersone aangebode stimuli (woorde) aant. woorde korrek waargeneem aant. woorde verkeerd waargeneem aant. woorde glad nie waargeneem nie proefpersone aangebode stimuli (woorde) aant. woorde korrek waargeneem neutrale woorde hakkelwoorde neutrale woorde hakkelwoorde ε 1 12 8 2 2 ε 2 12 9 2 1 ε 3 12 11 1 ε 4 12 8 2 1 1 ε 5 12 10 1 1 tabel iv getal stimuluswoorde wat ouditief aangebied word tydens die spontane spraak van hakkelaars. 46 ii. 60 =76,7% van die stimuliwoorde is korrek waargeneem; 1 1 ' iii. 60 = 20% is foutief waargeneem; _2 iv. 60 = 3,33% is glad nie waargeneem nie; v. 7 hakkel en 7 neutrale woorde is defektief waargeneem. a f l e i d i n g s 1. as bogenoemde syfers in aanmerking geneem word wil dit voorkom asof daar nie in hierdie eksperiment bewys gelewer is van die bestaan van onwaarneembaarheid tydens die hakkelperiode nie. 2. dit skyn asof die aard van stimulus (hakkel/neutrale woord), met betrekking tot sy emosionele kwaliteit nie met onwaarneembaarheid in verband gebring kan word nie. voor die uitvoering van die eksperiment is gemeen dat hierdie gegewens beslis 'n verskil sal vertoon en wel t.o.v., 'n groter onwaarneembaarheid van hakkelwoorde. die voorlopige hipotese wat gestel is, het verklaar dat onwaarneembaarheid verband kan hou met die aspek van perseptuele weerstand — d.w.s., onwaarneembaarheid van dreigende stimuli wat bo die drempel aangebied word. / bogenoemde gegewens kan op hierdie stadium nie die hipotese verwerp of aanvaar nie, aangesien die steekproef te klein was. oor die algemeen egter wil dit voorkom asof die hakkelaar nie onwaarneembaarheid ervaar tydens die hakkelperiode vir betekenisvolle spraakstimuli nie. 3. hierdie resultate is egter nie finaal nie, veral as in aanmerking geneem word dat alle proefpersone tydens die verloop van die eksperiment verklaar het: ! a) dat dit moeilik was om die woorde te hoor; journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ' kommunikatiefgefundeerde ondersoek 51 b) dat hulle dit oor die algemeen moeilik vind om stimuli waar te neem tydens die hakkelperiode. bibliografie 1. banreti fuchs, k.m. (1964)problemen der subliminale perceptie. von gorcum en comp. nederland. 2. davids, a. (1956) past experience and present personality disposition as determinants of selective auditory memory/, of personality. 25, no. 1, 19-26. 3. eisenson, j. (ed) (1958)stuttering: a symposium. harper and row, publishers, new york. 4. freund, h. (1966) psychopathology and the problem of stuttering. charles c. thomas, illinois. 5. froeschels and rieber, r.w. (1963) the problem of auditory and visual imperceptivity in stutterers, folia phoniatrica, 15, no. 1, 13-19. 6. howie, d. (1952) perceptual defense psychological review. 59, 308-315. 7. mussen, p.h. rosenzweig, m.r. (1969) annual review of psychology. 20. 8. selected papers of emil froeschels. (1964) north-holland publishing co. amsterdam, 1964. 9. sapir, e. (1949) language, harcourt, bruce & co. tydskrif van diesuid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) minimal closure in cleft lip and palate surgery d . h . walker, f . r . c . s . senior plastic surgeon, johannesburg hospital it is a frequently accepted surgical principle that one should do as much as possible for any one patient at a given operation. cutting across this is the slightly mocking injunction of the late sir harold gillies: "never do today what you can legitimately put off until tomorrow." is this an "easy o u t " for actually or potentially lazy surgeons or can the patient possibly benefit from this startling proposition? after careful thought, painstaking cephalometric measurements and long periods of follow-up and evaluation, we have an answer, at least in part, from slaughter and his associates in chicago, relating to operations and other treatment in patients with cleft lip and palate. this work is not new (papers were published in 1949, 1954 and 1958) but forgotten or disbelieved or neglected for a variety of reasons. among these reasons is probably the understandable feeling of satisfaction at the appearance of completeness in the early period after operation. historical aspects. faced with the frightening cavities of a complete bilateral cleft lip and palate, a sense of panic sometimes intrudes and the cry goes up to close it quickly and close it all in a desperate hurry. t h i s used to be attempted of course, as early as the sixteenth century and arthur barsky, a new york plastic surgeon, reports that cleft lip surgery is said to have been performed in china in the chin dynasty (a.d. 229-317) but it is apparently almost impossible to separate myth and fact in accounts of early chinese surgery. it is generally concluded that pierre franco's account of surgical closure of cleft lips published in the mid-sixteenth century is probably the first published. how much credit is due to the great ambroise pare, the dominating figure in french surgery at that time, and how much to franco, his apparently more modest contemporary, is unknown. at first the lip operation was a crude pulling together of the cleft margins and transfixion by pins, the whole being held by a figure of eight' of waxed thread wrapped around the projecting ends of the pins about eight or ten times.^ where the gap was wide franco advised relaxing incisions inside the mouth. t h e r e was little advance on this until the nineteenth century and closure of the cleft palate was almost impossible until general anaesthesia developed in the 19th century. with the patient unconscious under an anaesthetic, it is regrettable to note that surgical violence in this field 1 increased. forcible manipulation and breaking of parts of the alveolar arch with savage displacement and journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 13, no. 1: may 1966 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) minimal closure in cleft lip and palate surgery 45 frequent amputation of the projecting premaxilla became common. ruthless undermining of soft tissues outside and inside the mouth enabled these soft parts to be brought together, but only many years later was it recognised that some patients, who had been subjected to such surgical barbarism had, in adult life, definite patterns of deformity and disproportion in their faces. t h i s was almost certainly due to the gross interference with blood supply of the bones forming the middle third of the facial skeleton. holdsworth records that as early as 1864 simon suggested that force was not necessary and that the pressure of the united lip was sufficient to reduce the projection in the infant. his suggestion does not seem to have been taken very seriously until julius wolff in 1886 advanced the same idea. he also recommended the closure of bilateral clefts in two stages. thereafter the body of conservative opinion grew, though the subperiosteal fracture or resection of the septum remained a common feature of operation in extreme cases. growth of the facial area. t h e probable normal growth stimulus for the central area of the face starts as far back as the base of the skull (fig. 1). t h e "driving force," as it were, proceeds in a long curved line (shown dotted), directed by the vomer and transmitted, ultimately, as forward growth of the tip of the nose and upper lip. t h e structures concerned in this midline growth plane also form the bony and cartilaginous septum between the two halves of the nose. fig. i . sagittal section of the skull. s i : skull interior. f s : frontal sinus. n b : nasal bone. u t : upper teeth. d : direction of growth of mid-third of face and tip of nose. v : vomer and nasal septum. fig. 2. anterior aspect of the skull. n b : nasal bones. n c : nasal cavity. v : front view of nasal septum (vomer). in effect then, a face grows not from where it appears to grow, for example, at the tip of the nose or by an advancing upper lip, but by development far behind and above these regions. a frontal view of the region affected by this process may be seen in fig. 2. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 13, nr. 1: mei 1966 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 d. η. walker no one knows the detailed plan of the blood supply to growth centres in these regions. nor is it known how much of the soft tissue can safely be stripped off the bone and moved elsewhere in operations for the closure of cleft lip and palate. carried to a literal and over-logical conclusion, one would be afraid to operate on a cleft lip or palate as long as any growth at all is going on. in practice it is believed that sufficient activity of the growth centres has taken place by about the age of four years to make any reasonably radical operation safe from the growth point of view. apart from sheer growth, there must be a consideration of what may be described as "moulding forces." these are essentially derived from muscle action in the face, lip, tongue, soft palate and pharynx. a diagrammatic representation of a normal nose, lip, palate and pharynx (fig. 3) helps to present the concept of a band of muscle in the lip constantly acting across the bone beneath the nose and the alveolar arch bearing the teeth. t h e arrows show the direction of forward growth, which is almost certainly modified by the muscle in two planes. it is also reasonable to believe t h a t : the muscle ring of the mouth, as represented in fig 3, constantly exerts similar forces in a third plane. journal the south african logopedic society, vol. 1, no. 1: may 1966 fig. 3. diagram of palate, lip and nose from below. m u l : muscle in upper lip. h p : hard palate. s p : soft palate. ρ : pharynx. m r : muscle ring. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) minimal closure in cleft l i p and palate surgery 47 farther back the muscle of the soft palate acts in a variety of directions as does that forming the pharynx. one comes to regard the muscular moulding forces of the lip, palate and pharynx as composed largely of two rings, a "mouth muscle ring" and a "palato-pharyngeal muscle ring." apart from the normal moulding forces, it is clear that there must be a tendency to unrestrained and unmodified growth in certain directions if these rings are open, interrupted or undeveloped. this, of course, is exactly the situation which pertains in a patient who has a cleft lip and palate. we now near a possible answer to our originally quoted suggestion from sir harold gillies. principles and practice. as in many awkward problems, the answer is a compromise. in this instance, the compromise takes the form of starting a process by a surgical operation, which we hope will be advanced and even concluded by natural processes. if we refer to fig. 4, it can readily be appreciated that joining muscle in the lip across the cleft must tend to produce a backward pressure on the major alveolar segment, as well as a tendency for the major segment to move towards the side of the cleft. t h i s possible movement can be taken advantage of not only surgically, but by means of an orthodontic appliance. t h i s latter method is probably the major advance since the plan was originally put forward by slaughter and his colleagues. slaughter's original views suggested that in wide clefts where it is almost impossible to get a normal appearance at the first operation, one should be satisfied with any joining of muscle across the cleft lip and gum. t h i s region is then left to the consequences of the surgical dictum that having put normal parts in as near as possible to the normal position, natural c © fig. 4. cleft lip and palate from below. l : lip. g : g u m . c : cleft. m r o : muscle ring open. m : muscle. fig. 5. residual fistula (f) in partly repaired cleft narrows later. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 13, nr. 1: mei 1966• r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 d. η. walker growth forces should be allowed to exert themselves for a period of years. if we now apply this principle to the cleft in the soft palate, whether it is part of a complete cleft or a cleft of the soft palate alone, it will be seen that the joining of the muscle in the region of the uvula or as far forward as can be done without undue tension, must exert all the force produced by an intact palato-pharyngeal muscular ring. however, this must not be taken literally as a representation of the short term result of joining a band of muscle across the lip cleft to complete as much as possible of the muscular ring in relation to the palate and pharynx. t h e intervening unclosed cleft is deliberately left, with the possible exception of closure of the anterior portion of the hard palate cleft by a vomerine flap. we must now attempt to visualise growth in about 3 places in different directions at the same time: 1. t h e forward growth of the middle third of the face already described, which will have the effect of lengthening the overall front to back dimension of the palate. 2. at the same time muscular forces in the lip are at work. 3. soft palate and pharynx muscles are working from side to side and in a circular manner. as a result of the interplay of these forces, it is believed that a large number of the residual elliptical clefts change their shape as the years go by. in the favourable case this change of shape takes the form of a long, narrow ellipse replacing the shorter, wider oval that remained immediately after the surgical closure in front of and behind it. (fig. 5.) this approach to the whole problem based on the anatomical and physiological features outlined, is being tried at our unit in the johannesburg children's hospital, with the assistance of our orthodontic colleagues, who are keeping a close watch on the shape of the residual cleft in children, who have had muscular closure in front of and behind this cleft. they are able to achieve quite satisfactory speech results with an obturator. this appliance has, of course, to be re-made fairly frequently as the child grows and as the shape of the residual central defect alters. this may, however, be a justifiable period of inconvenience in view of the fact that we hope by this method, to avoid the sometimes severe disproportion in growth of the facial skeleton, which is likely to follow the radical operation at an early age. t h e final point is usually raised in the question of when, if at all, any operation may be needed. we believe that nearly all patients need another operation for eventual closure of the central residual cleft. this may be safely left until the age of 12 or 14 years, when one hopes that the edges of the cleft will be so closed together that the final operation will be relatively easy to perform and cause little distress for the patient. opsomming hoewel slaughter en sy kollegas reeds in 1949, 1954, 1958 'n meer konserwatiewe benadering aanbeveel het by die behandeling van lipen verhemeltesplete is hulle werk vergeet of gei'gnoreer, en in elk geval nie algemeen aanvaar nie. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. z.1, no. 1: may 1966 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) minimal closure in cleft lip and palate surgery 49 die oorspronklike operasies op gesplete lip en verhemelte was baie radikaal en die katastrofiese gevolge hiervan het dikwels eers na jare tot openbaring gekom in die vorm van wanontwikkeling van die gesigskelet met gevolglike disproporsie van 'n kenmerkende aard. hierdie toestand was byna sonder twyfel die gevolg van belemmerde bloed toevoer na die ontwikkelende skelet in die middelste derde van die gesig. geleidelik het 'n konserwatiewe benadering egter begin posvat en is die belangrikheid besef van 'n gehegde lip oor 'n onderliggende spleet om die vervorming van abnormale strukture te bewerkstellig. hierdie werking is die gevolg van die kragte wat uitgeoefen word deur die palato-faringeale, en mondspierkringe in drie vlakke op omliggende benige dele. wanneer die spierkringe defek is weens 'n spleet vind ongebonde groei plaas, en die gevolge word duidelik gesien in sommige volwasse persone met 'n lipof verhemeltespleet. slaughter se benadering kom daarop neer dat die spierkring op 'n vroee stadium herstel word met minimale versteuring van groeiende weefsel. dit gee dan die spleet 'n kans om nouer te word saam met die normaalontwikkelende groeiende skelet. op 'n latere stadium word die spleet dan finaal geheg. hierdie vorm van behandeling word tans op die proef gestel in die johannesburgse kinderhospitaal in samewerking met die ortodontiese afdeling. in gevalle met erge verplasing van die premaxilla word kort na geboorte 'n prostese gemaak wat druk daarop uitoefen om dit terug te druk na die versteurde alveolere boog voordat die lipspierkring gesluit word, 'n obturator-plaat word gedra nadat die sagte verhemelte spierkring geheg .is tot op die stadium wanneer die finale operasie uitgevoer word op 'n „veilige" ouderdom van 12 tot 14 jaar of meer. i would like to thank mr. h. c. de wet for the afrikaans summary of this paper. references slaughter, w. b. and brodie, a. g. (1949): plast. reconstr. surg., 4, 311. slaughter, w. b. and pruzansky, s. (1954): plast. reconstr. surg., 13, 341. slaughter, w. b. and pruzansky, s. (1959): the rationale for velar closure as a primary procedure in the repair of cleft palate defects. plast. reconstr. surg., 23, 301. holdsworth (1957): cleft lip and palate. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 13, nr. 1: mei 1966 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) glossectomy b y diana m. whiting. b. a. log. (rand) & c.s.t. in recent years several patients who have undergone partial or total glossectomy have been referred to us for help in re-establishing adequate speech. i have been unable to find references to this type of case in available speech journals. i wish to present therefore, a case of total glossectomy followed by comments on partial glossectomy and glossectomy plus total mandibulectomy based on my own limited clinical experience. total glossectomy — a case history. h.w. was an afrikaans speaking coloured male 56 years of age. he was illiterate. medical history: in 1956 he presented to the radiotherapy department with a carcinoma of the tongue and received a course of radiotherapy. he was followed up· regularly and biopsies in june 1957 and august 1959 revealed no signs of malignancy. he remained well until november 1962 when he presented with a hard ulcerating lesion of the left tip of the tongue 2.5 x 1.5 x 1.5 cms. arrangements were made for his admission to hospital for the excision of this lesion but the patient failed to turn up. he reappeared in january, 1963, the lesion having more than doubled its size. on 5th feb. 1963 .a total glossectomy was performed and 10 days later the patient was transferred to a convalescent home. on 8th march, 1963 he was followed up in the radiotherapy department who reported "appears satisfactory but cannot swallow" and referred him for speech therapy. on examination the patient still had a nasal feeding tubes in situ. (he administered his own nasal feeds at the required intervals.) he was edentulous. hej could not swallow and had difficulty in directing his saliva into the oesophagus resulting in frequent clearing of his throat and coughing to relieve glottal irritation. his soft palate functioned normally and his lips were very mobile. his speech attempts were almost unintelligible. resonance was markedly distorted, but all vowels and diphthongs were distinguishable when imitated in isolation. consonants p, b, m were normal. he had already developed an accoustically corrent η and his t and d were distinguishable accoustically from ρ and b; f and ν were approximately correct; k and fricative g were glottal; s and ζ were absebt altogether. all other consonants were distorted, r and 1 causing perhaps the most difficulty. treatment was directed towards:1. teaching him to swallow by watching, feeling and imitating the raising and lowering of the larynx during swallowing. he was soon able to do this and was very anxious to resume oral feeding. within a week he could manage porridge and soup and the nasal tube was removed. 2. developing the flexibility of his lips and thereby assisting their adaptation as prime articulators for all consonant sound. 3. teaching him accoustically adequate compensatory articulation for all consonants previously made with the tongue. he.achieved spontaneously correct f, v, n, t and d sounds. he acquired excellent s and ζ sounds, but had more difficulty in incorporating these into spontaneous speech. he produced an adequate 1 and untrilled r. all these consonants were produced with the lips. his spontaneous compensation of the glottal stop fork and fricative g added to the glottal irritations caused by his difficulty in directing fluid and food into his oesophagus. a rasping hoarseness was resulting. it was essential to reduce laryngeal irritation, but pharyngeal substitutes were not possible being a total glossectomy. he was encouraged to compensate labially. the sound 25 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) he achieved accoustically approximated a t rather than k. this, however, reduced vocal abuse and his vocal tone improved. 4. improving vocal tone and resonance. all vowels and diphthongs were readily distinguishable but resonance remained markedly distorted. treatment was discontinued after about six weeks the patient having returned to his home. he regarded his speech as adequate saying " a s hulle vir my mooi luister kan hulle vir my verstaan" (if they listen to me carefully they can understand me). partial glossectomy. vlinical experience with 3 cases of partial glossectomy one involving half the anterior third, one the whole anterior third and one the whole anterior half of the tongue has shown how readily and with very little help such patients can adopt articulatory positions to produce normal speech. total glossectomy and mandibulectomy. i have had the opportunity to observe and assist one such patient during several stages of his rehabilitation, which is not yet complete. this patient is an afrikaans speaking illiterate coloured male in his fifties. he underwent the total removal of his tongue and mandible and associated soft tissue with the exception of the lower lip which was disected out along its lower border and attached temporarily to the hyoid bone. a pedicle was raised from his abdomen and used to reconstruct the lower half of face and the lower lip was returned to the appropriate position. the only mobile articulator for consonant sounds this patient has is his upper lip. this has become very mobile. he achieves adequate p, b, m, f, v, t, d, η sounds and a very creditable s sound, r and 1 are distorted, k and fricative g are glottal. with the help of gesture and personality he makes himself understood. summary. experience with this limited number of patients seems to indicate that we can expect the normal speaker, who has to undergo removal of part or the whole of his tongue, to re-establish normal or adequate oral communication fairly rapidly post-operatively. this readjustment may be largely spontaneous requiring help primarily in finding compensatory articulation for sibilants and velar plosives and fricatives. opsomming. ondervinding met hierdie beperkte aantal pasiente dui blykbaar aan dat ons die herbevestiging van normale of bev fedigende mondelinge kommunikasie redelik spoedig kan verwag van 'n normale spreker wat die gedeeltelike of algehale verwydering van sy tong operatief moes ondergaan. hiefdie heraanpassing mag grootliks spontaan wees, maar hulp en leiding is nodig in verband met kompensatoriese artikulasiebewegings vir sisklanke, velare plosiewe en frikatiewe klanke. 26 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'ν geval van kinderafasie ε . swiegers, b.a.log. (rand) and i . c . uys, b.a.log. (rand). pretoria hierdie artikel beskrywe 'n interessante geval van kinderafasie, wat deur 'n spraakterapeut behandel is: soos dikwels gebeur, is hierdie ook 'n geval waar diagnose bemoeilik word deur verskeie simptoomkomplekse en daar word dus ook hier staat gemaak op die bydraes van 'n span van deskundiges, o.a. 'n neuroloog, spraakterapeut, kleuterskoolonderwyseres, kliniese kindersielkundige en die ouers. om die nodige duidelikheid te verkry sal die verslae van die verskillende instansies eers kortliks gegee word, en daarna 'n opsomming van die habilitasieprogram, wat gevolg word deur die resultate en verdere aanbevelings. gevalsgeskiedenis naam: pieter fourie. ouderdom: 4 jaar. probleem: swak spraak, stadige ontwikkeling en lompheid. ontwikkelingsgeskiedenis. swangerskap: nege maande swangerskap. geen abnormaliteite is opgemerk nie. geboorte: induksie is toegedien. een dag na geboorte het die pasient swart koorsblare ontwikkel, maar dit het skynbaar nie gedui op 'n patologiese bloedtoestand nie. die pasient het ook 'n ligte graad van geelsug gehad. voeding: hy \yas slaperig en lui en het suigprobleme getoon. hy het later die suigprobleem redelik oorkom, maar probleme ondervind met die inname van harde voedsel. die slukproses het deurgaans probleme opgelewer. mylpale. sit: 5 maande; kruip: 10 maande loop: 18 maande. spraakontwikkeling: babbel: pieter was 'n baie stil baba. hy het min gebabbel en min reaksies op spraakstimuli getoon. dit het gelyk asof hy nie kan hoor nie. spraak: op een jaar het hy „ma-ma" en „pa-pa" gese, maar geen nuwe woorde gese gedurende die volgende jaar nie. op vier jarige ouderdom kon hy 'n paar woorde betekenisvol gebruik, maar nog geen sinne nie. later het hy gesprekke begin naboots d.m.v. jargon. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.14, nr. 1: sept. 6 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 ε. swiegers and i. c. uys. toiletgewoontes: dit is.nog nie aangeleer nie. mediese geskiedenis. hy het die volgende siektes al redelik dikwels opgedoen: kroep, bronchitis, en griep. op twee jarige ouderdom het hy 'n baie hoe koors ontwikkel en is met · penicillin behandel. die rede vir die koors kon nie vasgestel word nie. familiegeskiedenis. sover vasgestel kon word, bestaan daar nie 'n soortgelyke probleem by enige familielid nie. indruk van die ouers. die ouers is beide baie intelligente, volwasse persone, wat a.g.v. hulle insig en objektiwiteif, 'n konsekwente program tuis sal kan uitvoer!. \ spraakterapeutiese ondersoek uiterlike voorkoms. 'n aantreklike, netjies seun; normaal gebou vir sy ouderdom. ·-, spraakmeganisme. tong: alle tongbewegings na links is defektief. in en uit bewegings is bevredigend, maar bewegings van kant tot kant is swak, stadig en onbeheersd. lippe: die pasient haal deur sy mond asem en die lippe is dus altyd oop. die lippe toon 'n gebrek aan tonus en, hoewel bewegings uitgevoer kan word, is dit stadig. kakebeen: hy kan die kakebeen lig en laat sak. tande: geen abnormaliteit nie. harde verhemelte: geen abnormaliteit nie. sagte verhemelte: geen abnormaliteit kan opgemerk word nie en hoewel die bewegings normaal blyk te wees, kan hy alleenlik snork, maar nie blaas nie. basiese funksies: die pasient kan blykbaar normaal sluk,"suig en kou. as gevolg van die oop mond kwyl hy soms. hy nuttig 'n normale dieet. asemhaling: geen afwyking is te bespeur by inof uitaseming nie. artikulasie: sy spraakpatroon word gekenmerk deur veelvuldige weglatings, verdraaiings, vervoegings en misartikulasies. as gevolg van die taal tekort is dit moeilik om die artikulasiefoute presies te bepaal, maar baie foutiewe klanknabootsings kom wel voor. hy ondervind ook moeilikheid met die plasing van die artikulasieorgane. stem: die stem is monotoon en ook effens hees. gehoor: die pasient reageer goed op growwe klanke en svrye-veld toetse. gehoorwaarneming blyk normaal te wees. verbale begrip: dit is baie swak. hy voer glad nie verbale bevele uit nie. dit kan egter ook toegeskryf word aan ander faktore. motoriese koordinasie. growwe bewegings: loopbewegingsj is lomp en hy sleep sy voete. hy kan glad nie hardloop nie en sy sin vir balans is swak. fyn bewegings: fynere bewegings van die hande, vingers, voete ens. is ook lomp en onderontwikkel vir sy ouderdom. hand-oog koordinasie: ook onderontwikkel vir sy ouderdom. journal of the south african-logopedic society, vol. 14, no'. 1: september 196η r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η geval van. kinderafasie 9 liggaamsvoorkeur. hand: links. oog: links. voet: links. intelligensie. vineland social maturity scale: (gegewens verkry van moeder.) sosiale ouderdomsvlak: ι jaar 9 maande. kronologiese ouderdom: 3 jaar 5 maande. formele intelligensietoetsing: dit was, weens die pasient se ouderdom en ontwikkeling nie moontlik om hom te toets nie, maar globaal gesien lyk dit of die pasient sub-normaal kan wees. vormpersepsie. die pasient kan voorwerpe en prente, wat hy ken, herken en .dan ook aandui op 'n verbale bevel. persepsieversteurings is wel teenwoordig en veral merkbaar m.b.t. ruimtelike orientasie. figuur-grond versteurings kom ook voor. spraak en taal. die pasient maak meestal van jargon gebruik, maar soms kan woorde herken word. die jargonspraak het ook 'n afwykende intonasiepatroon en veral die klinkers het 'n eienaardige kwaliteit. hy boots graag woorde en kort sinne na en met die nabootsing is die intonasiepatroon ook dikwels korrek. hierdie nabootsings mag moontlik op 'n vorm van perseverasie dui, aangesien dit redelik gereeld,voorkom en nie sin vol gebruik word nie. hy het 'n baie beperkte aktiewe woordeskat wat hy gebruik, maar dit mag wees dat sy begrip van gesproke taal verder ontwikkel is. reaksies. pieter reageer baie onkonsekwent en is uiters hiperaktief. as gevolg hiervan is hy moeilik dissiplineerbaar en kan hy nie lank genoeg op 'n gegewe stimulus konsentreer vir die nodige leerproses om plaas te vind nie. hy is egter in staat om vir langer tye te konsentreer op stimuli waarin hy belangstel, maar dit mag ook moontlik op 'n vorm van perseverasie dui. hy word baie gou moeg en verveeld, maar wanneer hy uitgerus en ontspanne is, lyk dit asof hy leergierig is en dan kan sy samewerking tog verkry word. persoonlikheid. hy is baie liefdevol en vriendelik en kontak word maklik opgebou. soms is hy egter baie gefrustreerd en moeilik hanteerbaar. neurologiese ondersoek algemene bevindings. 'n noukeurige neurologiese ondersoek het aan die lig gebring dat daar 'n neurologiese abnormaliteit teenwoordig is aan die regterkant van die liggaam. dit neem die vorm aan van 'n geringe verswakking van die gesigspiere en ook 'n verhoging van die reflekse. die linkerbeen toon 'n algemene verswakking en wanneer die pasient loop draai die linkervoet na binne. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 196η r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 ε. swiegers and i. c. uys elektroenkefalografiese bevindings. eerste verslag die pasient is in 'n toestand van slaap getoets. resultate: 1. veelvuldige linker frontale en fronto-temporale piekontladings en ook skerp golwe kom voor. hierdie abnormaliteite kom dikwels ook bilateraal voor. 2. interhemisferiese asinkronismes en aktiwiteit van 'n baie lae frekwensie, wat maksimaal is in die linker temporale en temporo-oksipitale areas, kom voor. die bevindings dui sterk op 'n gelokaliseerde abnormaliteit in die linker frontale en fronto-temporale areas, wat moontlik geassosieer is met verdere diffuse disfunksie in die temporale en temporo-oksipitale lobbe van dieselfde hemisfeer. tweede verslag die pasient is in 'n wakker toestand getoets. resultate: 1. 'n dominante frekwensie van 7 s/sek. (gemiddeld), wat effens onder die omvang vir sy ouderdom is, kom voor. 2. 'n buitengewone regter frontale versteuring van middelmatige spanning teen 6 s/sek. is opgemerk. 3. een fokale middelmatige poolspanningsparoksisme (6 s/sek.) het gedurende fotiese stimulasie by 30 f/sek. voorgekom. die bevindings dui op 'n disfunksie van die linker parieto-temporale area, wat ook die regter frontale area tot 'n mate betrek. diagnose * die pasient se defektiewe intellektuele, en spraakontwikkeling kan toegeskryf word aan 'n organiese breinbesering. dit is moeilik om die oorsaak van die breinbesering presies vas te stel, maar dit mag wees a.g.v. bloeding net na geboorte of a.g.v. 'n te vinnige geboorte. dit is opmerklik dat die pasient linkshandig is en, alhoewel dit toegeskryf kan word aan die besering aan die linker serebrale hemisfeer, is daar ook 'n oorerwingsfaktor hier betrokke, daar die moeder en een van die ander kinders ook linkshandig is. die kliniese ondersoek dui egter op besering van die linker serebrale hemisfeer en dit lyk asof dit die dominante hemisfeer is. die eeg dui daarop dat die afwyking maksimaal in die linker frontale area voorkom en dit mag impliseer dat die pasient beserings van die spraakareas opgedoen het, wat die vertraagde spraaken intellektuele ontwikkeling tot gevolg het. indien die besering van die spraakareas geassosieer is met 'n baie meer omvattende probleem, bv. die versteuring van die basiese intellektuele funksies, soos geheue, abstrakte redenasievermoe ens., sal die prognose swak wees. indien net die spraakareas aangetas is, sal die prognose vir spraakterapie moontlik goed wees. journal of the south africanlogopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 196 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η geval van kinderafasie aanbevelings 1. hertoetsing oor 6 maande. 2. ouerleiding en bekendstelling met die probleem. 3. spraakterapie. 4. chemoterapie d.m.v. die toediening van encephabol vir 'n toetstydperk. 5. kleuterskool. · kleuterskoolverslag eerste verslag emosionele staat. hy het homself goed aangepas by die kleuterskool, waar hy gewoonlik ontspanne en gelukkig is. soms raak hy ontsteld as hy geterg word, of as hy ander kinders se speelgoed wil he. sosiale ontwikkeling. hy speel eerder alleen of met een ander kind, maar vir kort periodes. hy is egter ge'interesseerd in groepspel. spelbelange. driewiele, sand en water, swaai, wendy-huis, boublokke, treintjies, klei en verf. algemene opmerkings. hy vind dit nog moeilik om te deel. hy is nog nie baie behulpsaam met roetine aktiwiteite nie, bv. om sy tas te bere en hande te was. hy neem baie goed deel aan die oggendkring en stories, maar kan nie vir lank konsentreer nie. tweede verslag emosionele staat. gelykmatig van geaardheid. raak alleenlik ontsteld as hy gedwarsboom word in sosiale spel. sosiale ontwikkeling. hy speel dikwels nog alleen, maar soms ook met een maat. dit is nog parallelle spel. spelbelange. hy speel baie graag met inpasblokke en met wiele, waarmee hy iets bou wat loop of draai. hy is baie oorspronklik en konstruktief hiermee. hy konsentreer lank hierop en hou homself besig. roetine aktiwiteite. hy wil nog nie altyd saamwerk nie. dit word egter verwag en daar word vriendelik, dog beslis opgetree. (miskien geniet hy in die stilte die aandag wat hy kry.) met oggendkring en storietyd sit hy nou stil en hy is ook dikwels gemteresseerd in prente en storieboekies. verstandelike ontwikkeling. hy hanteer die potlood, kryt en kwas nog lomp. hy herhaal nie meer so graag woorde en sinne soos in die verlede nie. algemene opmerkings. hy het op 'n stadium baie goed gevorder, maar nou het hy 'n plato bereik. dit kan moontlik aan sy liggaamlike toestand toegeskryf word, omdat hy baie afwesig was weens siekte. spraakterapeutiese program doelstellings. 1. die verbetering van dissipline om die hiperaktiwiteit teen te werk en die leerproses te bevorder. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 196 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ε. swiegers and i . c . uys 2. die verbetering van konsentrasie en aandag. 3. motivering om spraak as 'n kommunikasiemiddel te aanvaar en te gebruik. 4. die opbou van 'n basiese woordeskat.. 5. die aanleer van element ere sinsbou en taalpatrone. 6. ouerleiding vir die versterking van die terapieprogram tuis. resultate. pieter het eers baie stadig gevorder en ontwikkel, maar na 'n paar maande was sy vordering so bevredigend dat verdere doelstellings in die vooruitsig gestel is. verdere doelstellings. 1. uitbreiding van die woordeskat na saamgestelde, selfs meer abstrakte woorde. 2. taalverbetering d.m.v. rympies en stories, wat ook. liggaamsritme en koordinasie sal verbeter. 3. verbetering van persepsie op die volgende vlakke: (a) gehoorpersepsie—d.m.v. klankstimulasie; die herkenning, interpretasie en toepassing daarvan. (b) visuele persepsie—d.m.v. visuele stimulasie bv. die herkenning van kleure, figure en vorms. (c) figuur-grond waarneming—d.m.v. gesig en gehoor stimuli. (1d) waarneming van ruimte en tyd en orientasie tot ruimte en tyd, ook met die doel om liggaamsbewegings en koordinasie te verbeter; (e) klassifikasie en kategorisering met behulp van items van die nebraska en goldstein-shearer toetse. (/) verbetering van reekswaarnemings. 4. vermindering van oormatige nabootsing en perseverasie. 5. spelterapie vir emosionele ontlading, sosialisering en ontwikkeling van abstrakte vermoens. 6. ouerleiding. resultate. pieter het goeie vordering getoon, hoewel 'n tipiese plato-leerkurwe tog voorkom. verbetering het op al die gebiede plaasgevind, maar hy is nog altyd redelik konkreetgebonde. spraak en taal. hy het nou 'n redelik uitgebreide woordeskat. sy begrip van gesproke taal is nog steeds beter as sy gebruik daarvan. sinsbou het egter baie verbeter. hy maak nou baie gebruik van egosentriese spraak en veral in die oplossing van probleme. ^ liggaamsritme en koordinasie. hierdie aspek het baie' verbeter. die verswakking van die linkerbeen is byna glad nie meer merkbaar nie. fynere hand-oog koordinasie het ook verbeter en hoewel hy nou redelik goed slaag met die hantering van bv. 'n potlood, is nog verdere verbetering op hierdie gebied nodig. die prognose blyk heeltemal gunstig te wees. persepsie. abstrahering is nog 'n groot probleem—hy is nog baie konkreetgebonde. visueleen gehoorspersepsie het verbeter. vormwaarneming, kategorisering en klankwaarneming het, relatief gesproke, die grootste verjournal of the south africanlogopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 196 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η geval van kinderafasie 13 betering getoon. ruimtelike orientasie lewer nog soms probleme op veral t.o.v. die liggaamsbeeld. 'n moontlike ligte vorm van disartrie mag die oorsaak hiervan wees. perseverasie. hy is glad nie meer so geneig om -die verbale spraak van 'n volwassene na te boots nie. hierdie vorm van perseverasie het dus afgeneem en tesame met hierdie verandering het die groter afhanklikheid van egosentriese spraak voorgekom. die gebruik van egosentriese spraak het weer bygedra tot die ontwikkeling van die hoer verstandsfunksies in die sin dat dit hom gehelp het met probleemoplossing en abstrakte orientasie. perseverasie kom egter nog in sy eie spraakpatroon voor, maar die jargon element het baie verminder. sosiale en emosionele ontwikkeling. pieter maak nou baie makliker kontak met ander mense, hoewel nog vir baie beperkte periodes. sekere gedragsprobleme het egter nou ontwikkel, soos woedebuie, jaloesie en in sommige opsigte, hiperinhibisie. dit mag egter 'n normale reaksie wees op die vordering wat reeds plaasgevind het. hy begryp nou beter wat om hom aangaan, maar sy beperkte insig in menseverhoudings veroorsaak nou gedragsprobleme. hy kan nog nie „deel" en „gee" nie, maar wil uitsluitlik „ontvang". aanbevelings. die volgende aanbevelings word gemaak, gegrond op die waarskynlike gunstige prognose en sy vordering tot dusver: 1. spraakterapie soos voorheen toegepas, maar gemik op die verdere ontwikkeling en verbetering van die verskillende vermoens en vaardighede. 2. arbeidsterapie met die oog op die verbetering van fisiese en verstandsaktiwiteite—veral a.g.v. die moontlike bestaan van 'n ligte graad van disartrie. 3. kleuterskool onderrig vir die versterking van terapie, sosialisering ens. 4. toetsing. hoewel dit tot dusver nog baie moeilik was om hom te toets, moet verdere toetse tog uitgevoer word om sy potensialiteite vas te stel en simptoomkomplekse uit te lig: (a) intelligensietoetsing. (b) persepsietoetsing. (c) persoonlikheidstoetsing. (waarskynlik eers op 'n latere stadium.) 5. beslissing oor plasing volgende jaar. verskillende toetse, o.a. ook skoolrypheidstoetse moet uitgevoer word om vas te stel of hy moontlik na 'n normale skool sal kan gaan en of plasing by 'n ander instansie, bv. die skool vir serebraalverlamdes, meer gewens is. opsommimg 'n interessante geval van kinderafasie word hier bespreek. volgens die neurologiese verslag blyk dit asof die breinletsel, a.g.v. serebrale bloeding net na geboorte, maksimaal voorkom in die linker frontale area, met 'n moontlike implisering van ander areas. die linker hemisfeer is blykbaar die dominante hemisfeer. volgens die spraakterapeutiese ondersoek, asook die gegewens verkry uit ander verslae, is 'n omvattende terapieprogram opgestel en uitgevoer. die resultate is baie bevredigend en die prognose blyk baie gunstig te wees. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 196 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 ε. swiegers and i. c. uys summary an interesting case of childhood aphasia has been discussed. neurological reports revealed brain injury resulting from post-natal cerebral bleeding. this appeared to be manifest in the left frontal area. the left hemisphere is apparently the dominant hemisphere. as a result of a very full investigation a comprehensive therapeutic programme was undertaken. the results have been satisfactory, indicating a favourable prognosis. journal of the south africanlogopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 196 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages e.o.j. westphal, m.a. (witwatersrand), ph.d. (lond.) head, department of african languages, university of cape town. summary the purpose of the paper is t o s h o w the restrictive effect of the temporal quantity [q] in s o u t h e r n bantu languages w h i c h , w i t h reference t o its c o m p o n e n t s , m a y also b e described as 'syllable'. all s o u n d changes occur w i t h i n iqj although there are harmonizing changes that e c h o the effects of [q] intersyllabically. opsomming die doel van hierdie studie is o m die b e p e r k e n d e uitwerking van die temporale kwantiteit [q] in die suidelike bantoetale aan te t o o n . met verwysing na die k o m p o n e n t e , kan [q] o o k beskryf word,as „lettergreep". alle klankveranderinge vind plaas b i n n e [q] alhoewel daar harmoniserende veranderinge is wat die effekte van [q] intersillabies weerspieel. on the occasion of this memorial publication dedicated to prof. p. de v. pienaar i wish to pay him a tribute . . . and to settle a 33 year old debt. in my undergraduate days i prepared an essay on sound change for him but i did not present it for scrutiny. instead i chose an easier alternative he had given me on another subject. what was in doubt at the time was the relationship between synchronic and diachronic linguistics, but since then the matter has arisen on frequent occasions and it is perhaps not inappropriate to present some parts of this essay on historical and contemporary sound change in southern bantu languages. on reading the important work by wallace l. chafe2 introducing his views on the nature of a 'semantic grammar' (meaning and the structure of language, 1970), i was struck by his repeated reference to a recapitulation of historical changes in the post-semantic processes he describes. he considers that certain changes, themselves being processes, should be described as processes also (chapters: 4.5,4.8, 5.4, 5.6, 20.6). in chomsky and halle's3 'syntactical grammar' such a description would not be 'explicit' in the required sense and phonological processes not yielding to 'feature phonetics' would be treated either as a phonological sub-component or would be relegated to the 'lexical component' hold-all. j my undergraduate essay was concerned with bantu consonants and consonant combinations which never occurred in given grammatical circumstances. for journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20 december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages 23 example, the phonology of class 9/10 words was a severely restricted phonology in which certain 'prime' sounds never occurred. this phenomenon could be handled easily enough in terms of junctions of the prefix nof classes 9/10 of nouns with the noun root, but, on the other hand, there were derivations from such class 9/10 words — side by side with the derivations into class 9/10 that preserved the class 9/10 shapes of the roots and not the 'prime' shape: ve.: -rofhe 'mud' as in lurofhe 11 'thin mud' -tope 'mud' as in matope 6 'mud' cf. thophe 9 'heavy mud', dope 5 'very heavy clay or mud' in the secondary root -tope -r-/-fh are replaced by -t-/-pi.e. not only the root syllable is affected but also the second syllable not directly in contact with the prefix n-. in venda the processes by which r/t/th/d are associated can perhaps be derived from extant examples, but this is not always the case in other languages. in such languages the native speakers' insistence that certain words are semantically associated cannot always be verified in such a direct m a n n e r . . . especially in the absence of suitable examples of sound-shift: so.: kgiitsana 'orphan' and mofutsana 'poor, friendless person' though a comparison with other languages will demonstrate that there is a relationship of -fu with -kguand that -fu may be derived from **hy and **fy (cb*ky and cb*py), there is only one example that will support this derivation viz.: so.:-fub^du'red'with cl. 9 form kgub^du but see also: -kgutshwane 'short', kgiidu 9 'tortoise' there is therefore clearly a hiatus in the sotho relationships between -fu and -kguand there are formations and back-formations that cannot be accounted for as processes, though the spontaneous nasalisation of certain combinations is in fact a process in sotho. thus: so. -kgutshwane 'short' cf. xh. -fuphi, -futshane cb* -kvpi so. kgudu 9/10 'tortoise' cf. xh. ufudu 11/10 cb* n-kydv clearly fhe following is a back-formation and not a regular form: so. mafura 6 'fat' is derived from so. ** ma-pvta and not from cb* ma-kyta the fact that historical changes and historical derivations play a part in spoken language is a factor that would have to be built into a 'syntactical' and (generative) grammar' in the form of a 'popular etymology component'.. the basis upon which such 'popular etymologies' may be weighed is that of the processes as revealed by regular and attestable shifts. this is directly in line note: ve = venda. so = southern sotho. the double asterisk indicates 'proto-sotho' or 'common sotho'. the single asterisk or cb followed by a single asterisk indicates 'common bantu'. cedillas and bars under vowels in sotho are vowel marks. the cedilla is also used to indicate dental sounds in venda since it is more convenient than the traditional dental sign (viz. the circumflex under [t] or id]). the phonological processes are indicated by the use of capitals with double or single lines through them. thus: ν = nasalisation, ν = de-nasalisation, v= vocalisation, v = devocalisation. new symbols are * and d where the sounds [t l l and [d ] are not analysed as affricates but as single lateral plosives. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 e.o.j. westphal with chafe's2 proposal — even if, as he suggests, sounds that do not occur in the spoken language have to be hypostatized. the southern bantu consonants could thus be arranged in a paradigm capturing their essential associations with a 'prime sound' somewhat as follows: prime sound +passive +diminutive +causative prime sound c+v cw+v c(w)+v cy+v nasal + c nc+v ncw+v nc(w)+v ncy+v nc ν nc+v ncw+v nc(w)+v ncy+v there are of course other processes that will be revealed in the paradigmatic statement for each language. in this statement nhas the meaning of 'plus nasalisation' or '+nasalisation' while nhas the meaning of 'minus nasalisation (of nc)' or '-nasalisation'. the treatment of the consonants in this manner reveals the basic importance of a unit within which fusion takes place. this will lead us to a statement of the entity q. the terms at the head of each column have the following significance: +p = passive (in which -wis an essential element), +d = diminutive (in which in general palatalisations and velarisations are only effected when -q.-, -ο-, and -ufollow), +c = causative. the columns marked +w do not merit special differentiation of passive and diminutive forms. s o t h o (ipa o r t h o g r a p h y ) : . +p +d +c f f/w tshw/f/w ph p/hw tshw/ b djw di/djw p' p / ' w ts'w/ m qw m qw . +p +d r rw tsh/ th thw / t / h +c s . 1/d lw ts'/dj ts' t' t'w t / ' / t / ' w =fc +w +w prime c nc nc prime c nc nc prime c nc nc residual sounds: clicks, / , dj-j relationship. though the fricatives are derivative sounds, they are not entirely and satisfactorily derivative and, so, merit separate statement: y η nw nw η ny . +w 4+w 4-thi+w ( n y ) ijijw +w h/0,h uw x-kxh +w (i}) ny prime c nc nc prime c nc nc s tsh (ts') ts' / t a j t / ' in this statement the ip a orthography has been modified for both typographical and phonetic reasons. the lateral fricative series (shown by the symbols [4-] and [d]) and the lateral plosives (shown by the symbols [fc] and [d]) are journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages 25 treated as single-movement sounds. the symbol [j] represents the voiced palatal fricative. xhosa (ipa o r t h o g r a p h y ) : +p +d +w . +w +w prime c ph t/hw t / w th +w ch 4+w kh/k' +w nc mp' nt/'w n t / ' nt' +w nc' nt' +w nk' +w nc' p' t / ' w t / ' t' +w c' k' +w prime c 6 c'w c' 1 +w j 4 +w φ, y +w nc mb ndjw n d j nd,l +w "j nd +w ng +w nc bh d j d j dh,lh +w jh dh +w gh +w prime c m nyw ny η +w (ny) +w nc m η +w ny +w nc mh nh +w nyh residual ounds: clicks, fricatives and vibrants (showr below): prime c f s / x nc mpf' nts' n t / ' x nc (k'r ) cts' ) (t / ' ) kx' (k'rh) (ts'h) ( t / ' h ) prime c (v) (r) (z) (y) (h) nc mv r nz n j h nc vh rh zh djh yh hh note: the bracketed pronunciations do not occur; only the tone-depressor pronunciations are found. no 'prime c' is a tone-depressor. though these paradigms do not reflect the ur-bantu or common-bantu soundshifts they will be seen to reflect the 'prime consonants' and the 'derived consonants' of each language, and, on comparison with other languages, of uror common-bantu. similar paradigms from some 40 or 50 other languages illustrate various degrees of complexity and various kinds of solution to the various problems posed in the sound-change situations, particularly those of the class 9/10 prefixes and of the passive. a full range of processes may be observed ranging from labialisations, alveolarisations, palatalisations, velarisations, devocalisations, vocalisations, and the like. in venda the processes of vocalisation and devocalisation and of nasalisation and denasalisation are active. in shona vocalisation is accompanied by implosion. what is not reflected in such statements is the underlying reason for the change which cannot simply be stated in these phonetic terms. this underlying reason may be hypostatised as a need for homogeneity within a particular kind of entity, the syllable. but such an entity is not provided by standard 'feature phonetics' or by any other phonology since it is assumed that whatever can be said about syllables is effectively stated in terms of their components . . . whatever these may be considered to be. in general the major elements incorporating whatever might be said about a hypothetical syllable are the vowels and consonants. yet, while two major divisions of speech tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 e.o.j. westphal sounds the vowels and the consonants are recognised there is no indication in modern theory why only 2 (perhaps 3) such major stratifications or divisions are made in the speech sounds of any given language . . . and not 4 or 5 or any number. the description of linearised speech sounds in whatever kind of grammatical segment they may occur (e.g. in-morphemes, words, phrases, sentences) lacks a fundamental coherence if it cannot be shown why vowels and consonants alternate in linearised sequences. phonologists arbitrarily and capriciouslyintroduce a major division of the speech sounds into vowels and consonants and then blandly state that they alternate in sequences such as c+v+c+v+ c+v etc. in both classical and modern (chiefly tg) grammar 'vowels' and 'consonants'.are regarded as axiomatic and there is no satisfactory explanation why they should be auditorily and functionally distinct nor is it shown how and where sound change occurs in a featureless sequence such as c+v+c+v +c+v. early attempts were made to introduce a coherence to phonological and phonetic descriptions by introducing the concept of 'a syllable', but with no marked success. the term has been used from middle english onwards, but in modern usage no reality could be given to the concept though it was felt to be a necessary concept. j.r. firth, according to f.r. palmer5 introduced the concept though he nullified its value by insisting that its behaviour could be adequately stated in terms of c+v sequences. chomsky and halle3 use the term though with a double meaning of '+vocalic' and '+syllabic consonant'. it is suggested by gillian brown1 that the terms 'syllabic' and 'consonantal' may be diversified as '+syllabic', '-syllabic', '+consonantal', and '-consonantal', though she also finds it necessary in her description to speak of 'syllables', 'syllable nuclei' and 'syllable onsets' while concluding that there is no justification for setting up a separate syllable generator. the result of this assortment is that nasal compounds are set up as the entity: +cons/+syll.. +cons/ -syll.. -cons/+syll, where the third element is the vowel. all this in a bantu language. the description emanates from chomsky and halle's3 treatment of the feature '+syllabic'. this they describe as one of their major class features: a 'sonorant' e.g. semivowels, glides, etc. b 'syllabic' e.g. vowels, syllabic consonants c 'consonantal' e.g. non-syllabic consonants etc. it is not difficult to see why the term 'syllable' is redundant in tg-grammar: it is built into the system of 'tg feature-phonology' as a synonym for what was earlier described as 'vocalic' and now carries the meaning of 'vocalic', 'syllabic consonant' and 'syllabic nucleus' or 'main syllable marker'. in the last sense it skirts the issue of syllable boundaries. this example, perhaps better than most, illustrates the opportunistic bases of tg 'feature phonology' . . . though those who have studied the 'evaluation measures' for determining 'complexity' by the 'markedness' or 'unmarkedness' of a sound, warn us not to come to a hasty conclusion on this point. | for those of us who would seek to establish auditory reality besides articujournal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages 27 eality and acoustic reality it is important to know whether 'syllables' l 3 t 0 r y "ch an auditory reality. from an auditory point of view it seems that chomsky and halle3 have created a cover term for 'vowel' and 'syllabic consonant' as follows: -fsyllabic vowel syllabic consonant w h e r e jt can be shown, as it can be in the languages of the sotho group, that syllabic consonants have syllabic quality while consisting of a '+conant' and a '+latent vowel' element, the terms 'syllabic' and 'vowel' are reversible in the formula and are therefore synonyms. one of them is clearly r e d u n d a n t . if this is so then the authors could equally well acknowledge the syllable as a 'generator' and describe their major class features as follows: a 'syllabic' = sonorant (+ further features provided) b '+syllabic' = vocalic c 'syllabic' = consonant the single feature '(+-)syllabic' together with a restatement of the cavity, stricture type, and stricture degree features would then provide an absolute as against a contrived measure of complexity. whatever the result of such an application, the concept of the 'syllable' has by no means been dispensed with and the contrived 'unmarkednesses' devised to secure the smallest number of marked features will not obscure the need for it. indeed it has been built into the tg phonology as indicated, though as a synonym for vocalic. the framework provided by chafe's vastly more promising 'symbolization rules' in his semantic grammar is more capable of absorbing the syllable as a symbol and as a generator in directional speech leading (a) from a semantic structure to a phonetic structure as the articulatory output and (b) from a phonetic structure to a semantic structure as the auditory input. as chomsky and halle's3 terminology will show on examination, the auditory input is not their main concern, and other writers (e.g. fudge 19734) have commented on it. in southern bantu languages there is a temporal measure which, because of its relative and non-absolute nature, is not clearly revealed in experimental analysis. it is against this measure that we may judge one speaker to speak more rapidly than another, that we may say that some segments are longer than others in a sentence, that we may say that statements have a longer termination than questions, that some vowels are double-pulse vowels while others are merely long or short. i shall term the temporal quantity [q]. [q] contains several pre-phonetic features... by which i mean that it is not a symbolic abstract in the sense of firth's c+v successions. the essential markers of [q] are shown as its nucleus [/q/] and as its boundaries [#q q#]: (1) q->#q,/q/,q# + (q) however, [q] has certain functions that are localisable to one or other component just as the essential nature of [q] maybe stated in terms of the qnucleus and may be observed in the periodic recurrence of this nucleus. the localisable functions (which include tone, stress, duration) cannot be stated in terms of [q] itself but rather in terms of the q-nuclei and q-boundaries. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 e.o.j. westphal nevertheless they are functions of iq] and not of the components. the components in the nucleus and in the boundaries may roughly be characterised as [v] = vowels and [c] = consonants and statement (1) may then be expanded as follows: (2) q ((c(v)c), (q) where [c] is a consonant to be further specified and is a boundary, and where [v] is a vowel to be further specified and is a nucleus, while the right-hand [q] marks the recurrences. the corresponding tree diagram incorporating statements (1) and (2) cannot be stated until the localisable functions of [q] are placed. we thus invest the components [c] and [v] with these functions as follows: (3) c -> (c +stress prominence) (c -stress prominence) v-> (v +tonal prominence) (v -tonal prominence) v-> (v -hength prominence) (v -length prominence) the tree diagram then has the form: (4) 9 q *,q' / q / ^ q * i i 1 ; ! ; c v c (with features of stress, tonality, duration and : ί i others localised on one or other of these com! 1 : ι ponents) s d ' ( w h e r e s = s t r e s s > t = tonality, d = duration) where the locking c # is absent the temporal quantity may be said to be open, and where it is present it may be said to be closed. a closed q does not of necessity bring about a' change in vowel quality though it will almost certainly bring about a reduction in the duration of v. the s, τ and d features are not always confined to the components on which they are based e.g. as in the depressor-consonants of xhosa which anticipate the function of the tonal nucleus, or as the locking function of the nasal in nasal compounds of xhosa where the nasal element has a distribution in the preceding [q]. i should perhaps say explicitly that i am here concerned with the orthographic representation of auditory (not articulatory) features. the premature exclusion of these auditory features will severely restrict discovery procedures that will show some languages to be 'syllable number sensitive' and that will show that certain types of [q] permit a very restricted phonology only e.g. the bantu noun prefixes which are limited to 3 vowel sounds and 7 consonant sounds. the temporal quantity has a rhythmic nature though it clearly is not an exact measure of time as in music. at least one experiment can be set up with an journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 29 syllable and sound change in southern bantu nguages • flow meter to demonstrate the articulator basis of [q]. a l f ° mnonential statement in terms of its nucleus and its boundaries, the i t has a c ° " ^ t w e e n t h e s e d u t i e s b e i n g t r u e distinction between vowels and differenc ^ ^ w i t h c o r e d u t i e s a r e v o w e i s o r vowel-like while all c°n sh7with boundary duties are consonants or consonant-like each will therefore have its special functions according to its position in [q j. rnfhm tonal functions (usually but not only stated in terms of its [v]-coml u j i ~ a s t r e s s functions (which may be expressed by the v-component fc h very often with the introduction of boundary phenomena), duration (always stated in terms of the v-component or of the syllabic consonant c o m p o n e n t ) , loudness (stated in terms of [q]). rut fol also has features that have in the past been stated as functions of the consonants perhaps rightly in the case of some languages but which could also be stated in terms of the v-components, or, in terms of the whole unit. such features are e.g. aspiration in a series such as ph/th/kh or pfh/tsh/kxh ejection in a series such as p'/t'/k' or pf'/ts'/kx' voicing in a series such as b/d/g or bv/dz/gx or combinations of these as e.g. voiced aspiration in a series such as bh/dh/gh . . . these features can probably be multiplied endlessly as special features of consonants but they can and do also function as features of the vowels or of the whole temporal quantity. in this latter interpretation they will also be seen to interfere with tonality and can therefore also be stated as c to v transition phenomena as in the bushman languages. the following paradigmatic statement suggests the complete range of features that may be linked, according to the languages, either with [c] or with [v] or with [q]. in the statement it should be noted that voicing is an additive feature affecting [c] while aspiration [+h], ejection [+'], and, strangely, implosion [+"], (a laryngeal closure feature), are seen as vowel onset features. the frictionless continuants (such as the laterals, the nasals, etc.) and the vibrants are sounds that do not usually offer a choice in voicing: they are always voiced though they may be de-vocalised in certain combinations. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e.oj. westphal (p) plosive (f) fricative (k) click clear +hv +'v +"v clear +hv +'v + ' v clear +hv +'v +"v ( 1 ) (2) (3) (4) (5) (6) (7) (8) (9) (10) 01) ρ ph p' φ f pfh pf' θ oh 0 ' t th t' · θ t th t' s tsh ts' / /h /' t s t§h t$' ! !h /' ch /, t / h t / ' c ch c' 9 (cph cq') f ^h φ' t th t' 4(t+h t4-') ii hh k kh k' x kxh kx' q x h +voicing b bh b' b* β v d dh d' j d 3h* d' d ζ d ? j j jh j' j ν ά dh d' 4 i gh g' g* x g g* κ / fi (n) nasal (l) lateral clear +h clear +h bvh bv' bo boh bo' (l) (2) (3) dzh dz' g/ g/h g/' ( 4 ) dtfi dz' g! g!h g!' (5) djh* dj' ( 6 ) (jyh jy') gφ gfti gf' (7) (d4h d4') g// g//h g//' (8) gxh gx' (9) (10) (11) (r) rolled (w/y) semivowel clear +h + ' m mh η 1 ή nh ϊ lh r rh <5i w wh (1) (2) (3) (4) n^ h τ yh (6) (7) n r (10) ( 1 1 ) footnote * ί = [ £ ]: 6 = [b]: d = [cf]: j = [3]: g = [g·] journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) and sound change in southern bantu anguages 31 notes ή errh mav be whispered in which case voicing is not an essential part a) a u o t h e r characters of the sound may then come to the fore and ° f «t^rehed upon for distinctiveness. h^the t r a n s i t i o n from c to v may be 'clear', 'aspirated' (+h),'ejective' (+'), aid pressed with implosion of c' (+*). these transitions may be affected by voicing. . c ) the auditory characters are the following: (p) plosion i.e. stop consonants (f) friction i.e. obstruent continuant sounds (k) click i.e. injective suction sounds (n) nasal (l) molar-lateral non-obstruent sounds (r) rolled or tapped sounds (w) semi-vowels though (n) (l), (r), and (w>sounds may be whispered their character is 'voiced' in a number of languages they may appear as'voice' in combinations with v-c transitions e.g. [nh] in olunhkumbi, an angolan language. d) the place of articulation is indicated as follows: (1) labial (lab) lip-front (2) denti-labial (del) lip-back (3) dental (den) tongue-tip (4) alveolar (alv) tongue-front (5) retroflex (ret) tongue-tip (6) prepalatal (prp) tongue-front (7) palatal (pal) ' tongue-back (8) palatal-lateral (lat) tongue-side (9) velar (vel) tongue-root (10) faucal-pharyngal (fau) (11) glottal (glo) (12) epiglottal (epi), not shown in charts. from the second column indicating the position of the moving parts (lips, tongue, uvulus, pharyngeal wall, epiglottis and. larynx) it will be seen that the fixed-position classification is incidental to control and that the moving-parts classification is much more to the point and possibly more economical to state. e) the palatal and palatal-lateral fricative sounds are bracketed in the chart since they are indistinguishable in practice from the relevant plosive sounds, cf.: [ch] and [c?h], [c'] and [c?'], etc. for 'voiced' [th] and [t-th], [t] and [t4-1, etc. for 'voiced' f) all compound sounds may be stated in terms of a double/treble involvement of the moving parts features thus e.g. [px], [tx], [tsx], [pk], [mi)j, and many more: tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 e.o.j. westphal lab. . pk +voiced ps p$ ρ / p5 px +aspirated φ s tfo qi/ ^g ^x +ejective del. fs f5 if alv. ts tx/tsx sx ppl. tf cx/t/x /x vel. kx g) since [q] is a compensatory unit (a unit limiting the extent of compensatory movements), it will follow that certain effects such as de-vocalisation, aspiration, etc. will be limited by it in the grammatical (morpho-phonemic)' processes. thus e.g. the de-vocalisation in sotho languages in class 9 may be compared with the vocalisation of voiceless plosives in herero. a specification in terms of this analysis will permit us to reduce the 'feature phonetics' of chomsky and halle3 to a mere 19 items, though, in order to do this, we must specify some selections as necessary selections and some as optional. thus e.g. 'voicing' is a necessary selection with vowels and the continuants while there is a 'voice choice' item with such distinctions as p/b, f/v, etc. with the above analysis the primary operators may be confined to (1) primary q-features (i.e. syllabic features) and (2) voice-production (glottal) features. all other features are 'modification features' and not 'production features'. in chomsky and halle3 the 'unmarked' features on which 'complexity' is determined are ambiguous in that they include both 'no entry' and 'minus' features. if full use of this convention is made then the features needed to specify any sound uniquely can be reduced from their 30 to a mere 19. the use of this convention is however contrary to their basic thinking which appears in the main to be that of 'articulatory control features'. if now we were to turn our attention from 'output' or articulatory phonetics to 'input' or auditory phonetics (leaving acoustic phonetics as a laboratory interest), and, if we were not to use the combined 'no entry/minus' unmarkedness, then the following would probably be a maximum of features required for an 'auditory control phonetics': 1.0 primary q-features 1 +syllabic nucleus +vowel -consonant 2 +syllabic diffusion +depressor c -syllabic c 3 +η (tone) prominence +h ' -l 4 +up-step +up-step -down-step 5 +duration -hong -'normal' journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages 33 +stress +terminal 6 +stress prominence 7 +terminal crumble 2.0 airstream features g +clear 9 +hard 10 +voice obligatory 11 +voice choice 12 +pharyngal production 3.0 moving parts/place of articulation features 13 +-labial +bilabial 14 +-frontal +interdental 14 +apical +retroflex 16 +-radical +palatal 17 +lateral (molar) +lateral-molar 18 +faucal +pharyngeal 19 20 21 22 +clear +ejected +vocalized +voiced sounds +pressed -'normal' -non-terminal -aspirated -injected -devocalized -unvoiced sounds -scraped -dentilabial -alveolar -prepalatal -velar -laryngeal 4.0 degree operators (for 3.0 and for 1.1) -front/-back = central -high/-low = mid +front +back +high +low 5.0 release type features 23 + ρ (plosive) 24 +f (fricative and affricate) 25 +κ (clicks) 26 +ν (nasal continuants, voice obligatory) 27 +l (laterals, voice obligatory) 28 +r (rolled sounds) 29 +w/y (semivowels, voice obligatory) with the combined 'no entry/minus' convention these 29 features could be drastically reduced. clearly some of the associated features require further description e.g. the reason why 'clear/aspirated' are associated is in order to distinguish 'clear/turbulent'. similarly the '+hard','+voice obligatory', and many other features can only really be clarified from demonstrations in specific languages..this is not the right place in which to demonstrate these specifications. we are here concerned with the statement of the limitations imposed upon sound-change by [q] in the bantu languages. for this purpose it was necessary to place the 1.0 'primary q-features' within an updated framework. this has been done primarily with the thought of eliminating the ambiguity arising out of the use of the term 'syllabic' to denote vowels and consonants and to show that vowelness and consonantness are secondary to syllabicity features. a further thought was to provide the necessary auditory basis for the statement of sound change. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 e.o.j. westphal sound change this description is concerned only with telescoped syllables in bantu languages of southern africa. it is not concerned with the harmonisation processes in which grammatical and syllabic environment and not telescoping play a part. the harmonising processes may be described as inter-syllabic changes and the following is an illustrative (and therefore incomplete) list of such changes: a) accumulations of labial/rounded sounds cause change: ssotho: mmutla/mebutla 3/4 'hare' cv+ [b]v cc_+ [ m ] v / . . . mo+bv mm+mv venda: mmphasi/miphasi 3/4 'hubbly-bubbly' cf. -$asi cv+ [φ]ν cc+nφν c£+ [ p h ] v / . . . mu+^v mm+phv b) pre-harmonic vowel processes: ssotho: horekisa 'to sell' cf. horeka 'to buy' -rekr e k / . . . '+high vowel' i.e. i/e/^/u/o/q where a/e/o are '+low vowel' ssotho: -fqtse 'b. finished' cf. -fela 'come to an end' -fje -*• -fe-/... '+latent-vowel consonant' i.e. s/ts/tsh/ny/ng/some n, etc. c) post-harmonic processes (vowel and consonant): sound changes that are determined by a preceding syllable include the choice of the 'applied' extension according to the root vowel e.g. -ira/ -era/-ina/-ena, -irira/-erera/-inina/-enena, -ura/-ora/-una/-ona, -urura/ -orora/-ununa/-onona, etc.; the choice of the final vowel in languages like herero where e.g. okumuna 'to see' appears as -munu in the environment '-(-present tense action'. there are also consonantal processes where a root syllable will affect secondary root syllables in reduplications e.g. venda: phalaphala 9/10 'sable antelope' +n$ala+n$ala cf. wsotho phalafala +nfalafala and shona mharapara +nparapara. this process is also reversed in venda: kupalapala 20/8 'small sable antelope' ku-n^ala-n^ala where '-n' represents 'denasalisation' several tonal processes and the typical 'concord system' of bantu languages may also be seen in this light as anticipatory or pre-harmonic processes and as subsequent or post-harmonic processes. we may now consider any utterance in a southern bantu language as a movement across a reticle in which the interstices are temporal quantities formed by c and v alternations. the threads not selected in this movement remain as contrastive elements with a semantic significance of their own. in this model of speech consonants and vowels are selectional items together with a host of 'features' presenting some 2000 specifiable and unique q-entities for an average language of 40 consonants and 5 vowels. we have already specified q as having the following behaviour: q c+v, (q) in the reticulated model of utterance we must specify q-sequences as q-cohesions in which any q has ordinal position and in which the total number of q is always stated. the ordinal position of q states whether it is a root syllable journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages 35 (with stress) and the count of the number of q states what prefix rules and what tonal rules shall be applied. in several languages monosyllabic stems require distinct prefixes and in almost all of them the number of syllables by which a word is reduced determines the tonal profile selected i.e. there is an order of deletion which is confined to the specified q-cohesions. we may thus also eliminate such rules as the following xhosa rule as an interim (inter-syllabic) rule: 2q (prefix) -*• 1 q / . . . '-(-polysyllabic stem' where the prefix contains the consonants [l], [b], [m] as in the prefixes ilicl. 5, ulucl. 11, ubu(v )̂ where v is a syllabic consonant with these marking conventions we may now describe sound-changes of a nonharmonising kind as q-reductions as follows: (a) examples of unchanged syllabic sta tus with soundchange 1q(c'+v) -*• 1q(c"+v) e.g. so. /i/-»· [ [l] ] / . . . '+e/e/e/a/o/q/o' [ [d]]/. . . '+i/u' e.g. so. / h h [ [s ]]/. . . '-h/e/e/e· [ [ h ] ] / . . . '+other vowels' [ wv . . . 'q2, q 3 . . . ' these are simply the allophonic rules in which the quality of a sound is determined by its immediate syllabic environment. these changes do not include the harmonisation processes. though there is an alternative rule which may be applied to the case where the vowels -e_and -onever occur before -s-, -ts-, -ny-, -ng-, -nng-, -tsh-, these environmental conditions are distributed over 2 syllables. they do not therefore have a place under this heading. (b) examples of unchanged syllabic status with added c 1. 2q(cv+v) remain 2q but insert a new -c-: e.g. herero oku+ -enda okuyenda, but also okuenda pattern: ol ' ' there are many patterns for dealing with juxtaposed vowels, even in herero where two vowels may be juxtaposed with no change and no insertion. cl. 14, ummcl. 3 and ummcl. 1 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 e.o.j. westphal (c) examples of reduced syllabic status but with length compensa ή on 1. 2q(cv+v) -»• cvv e.g. ndonga oka+iga -»• okiiga oka+uli -»• okuuli okayiga okayuli oka+elo -»• okeelo oka+oko ·-»• okooko okayelo / okayoko oka+anda -*• okaanda okayandawhichever pattern of juxtaposition is employed there is either no loss of syllabic status or there is durational compensation. examples of reduced syllabic status but with tonal compensation 2. 2q(cv+v) 1 q(cv) with tonal compensation e.g. venda: **ma+ito->• mato'eyes'(note: in all other circumstances the noun prefix is l) examples of syllabic reduction but with conversion of v ^ c or with total loss of one v: 3. 2q(cv+v) -*• lq(ccv) e.g. xhosa: uku+enza -*• ukwenza uku+akha -»• ukwakha 4. 2q(cv+v) -*• 1 q(cgv)/... 'where two rounded vowels follow each other, but only in combination with c' e.g. xhosa: uku+ona -»• uk^ona ->• ukona a similar process may be observed in herero where a labial consonant is incompatible with a rounded vowel e.g. herero cl. 14 prefix: o^u-»• ou5. 2q(cv+v) 1 ( c c v ) / . . . 'where all but rounded vowels follow' e.g. xhosa possessive concord: si+a -»• saand b^+a -»• ba-, but lu+a -*• lwaexcept b^l+a ->• ba(d) examples of syllabic reduction with vowel coalescence / there are many examples of this phenomenon which also involves a latent vowel and includes the substitution of an invariable vowel. 1. xhosa" (coalescence) na+imiumu-»• neminomu\ ama-· ^nama-^ 2. shona (latent vowel, harmonic) ; na+mimu-»• neminomus n v s m a / / / ^ n a m a ^ journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages 37 3. shona (vowel substitution) na+mimu-»• neminemu^ ^ m a ^ ^ ^ nena-/ 4. xhosa (syllabic vowel-harmonisation) there are numerous examples in many bantu languages of the assimilation of a suffixal element though this is done in such a way as to preserve the derivations: locative ** -(i)ni ci+ini cini cu+ini ->• cwini ce+ini -»• ceni co+ini cweni ca+ini ceni the bracketed vowel **-(i) is therefore a variable vowel which is i / . . . 'high vowel' -e-/ . . . 'low vowel' and which is preceded by the element w / . . . 'back vowel' (e) examples of reduced syllabic status but with vocalic penetration of c with these examples we now return to the consonantal paradigms for bantu languages. the most varied processes may be found in both suffixal and in prefixal positions. these processes also include consonantal modifications that are not always immediately apparent as changes resembling the allophonic rules but different from them in that they have to be established statistically and that some of them have to be described as the result of the juxtaposition of a vowel-sensitive consonant with certain vowels. though prefixal examples exist the vocalic penetration of consonants is far more frequently found in suffixal positions. nd. hamantu 'it is not a person' cf. ha+^muntu haantu 'it is not people' cf. ha+^antu haiyelo 'it is not doors' cf. ha+/iyelo hauyelo 'it is not small doors' cf. ha+^uyelo hangombe 'it is not a cow' cf. ha+^ngombe haongombe 'it is not cows' cf. ha+^ongombe xh. andinabantwana ί have no children' cf. andina+^bantwana andinasihlalo ί have no seat' cf. andina+/sihlalo andinasana ί have no baby' cf. andina+iisana andinankomo ί have no cow' cf. andina+/nkomo ί have no cows' andina+^nkomo andinamalume ί have no uncle' cf. andina+tfmalume ί have no uncles' andina+^malume nifuna bani? 'whom do you want' cf. nifuna ylbani? nifuna ^ b a n i ? nifuna ntoni? 'what do you want?' cf. nifuna /ntoni? lenkomo 'this cow' cf. le+jnkomo ' ezinkomo 'these cows' cf. ezi+j(inkomo lamadoda 'these men' cf. la+^madoda tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 e.o.j. westphal ababantu 'these people' cf. aba+^ibantu abafuni zihlalo zintathu uzithengileyo 'they don't want the three chairs you bought' cf. abafuni /zihlalo ^zintathu i^(si+u)zithengileyo. these xhosa examples have an alternative interpretation in the construction of two parallel series of prefixes, one a 'free prefix' and the other a 'junction (or bound) prefix' (f) 2q 1q with fusion (coalescence), harmonic change (coalescence), and substitution these rules are best illustrated with the prefixal element na'and, together with'. but also see possessives, nga'with' xh., etc. etc. 1. 2q(cv'+v") 1 q(cv"') + fusion of two vowels xh. nomntu 'and the person' na+i +u| -> neno-, cf. na+umntu \ +e +q nesihlalo 'and the chair' \ _ j t 2 cf. na+isihlalo nezintle 'and the good ones' cf. na+ezintle nomhle 'and the good one' cf. na+omhle 2. 1 q(cv') 1q(cv") + substitution of harmonic vowel (for -a-) sh. nomunhu 'and the person' na+ci na+cu cf. na+munhu navanhu 'and the people' cf. na+vanhu nemiti 'and the trees' cf. na+miti 3. 1 q(cv') -> 1 q(cv") + substitution of invariable vowel (for -a-) sh. nemunhu 'and the person' na+ci na+cu ne ne nevanhu 'and the people' nemiti 'and the trees' \ na+ca / \ ne. 4. 2q(cv++v") 1 q(cc.'v'") 1q(cv"') there are many other sequences of vowels than those of (a+v). these are too numerous to list from the various languages with vcv-prefixes and only one is brought here from the locative forms of nouns in xhosa loc. xh. **(i)ni (ci+ini cu+) -> icini cwinij \ceni cwenj/ ceni umthi 'tree' emthini, abantu 'people' ebaritwini, intonga 'stick' entongweni, umdlalo 'game' emdlalweni, indlebe 'ear' eridlebeni. the bracketed vowel **(i) is therefore a variable vowel which is determined as i / . . . '+high vowel' e / . . . '+low vowel' \ and which is preceded by ; w / . . . '+back vowel' journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages 39 (g) 2q ->• 1q with modification of consonants and with vowel penetration. with these examples we now return to the consonantal paradigms for the bantu languages. the most varied processes may be found in both prefixal (e.g. cl. 14 utywala 'beer' in xh.) and suffixal (e.g. diminutive, passive, locative) positions. these processes of consonantal change may also include consonantal modifications that are not always immediately apparent. they resemble the allophonic rules but differ from them in that they have to be established statistically and by the absence of certain vowel sequences after given consonants. these changes all have the pattern of: 2 q ( c ' v ' + v " / c v " ) 1 q ( c " 1 v " / c v " ) in which c " is the modified c the modifications are of the following kinds: labialisation: c(t)+v) cf. ve. retroflexion alveolarisation c(a)+v) cf. so. lefifi/lefitshwana retroflexion c(ft)+v) cf. ve. ulisa/uliswa [s] palatalisation c(p)+v) cf. xh. umlambo/umlanjana velarisation c(w)+v) cf. so. molamu/molangwana the following are manner of articulation modifications: implosivisation c(=)+v) cf. sh. dombo/matombo vocalisation c(v)+v) cf. ve. dombo/matombo devocalisation c(v)+v) cf. xh. **ubu+ani -* utyani these modifications are far too numerous to describe in detail in this paper but they are processes that will generate a great many sets of new consonants. a detailed statement of one example will suffice to illustrate these modifications: xh. umlambo+ana/umlanjana dim. 'river' <^u-mm-la:-mbo + ana-»· ^u-mm-la-mbw+a-na ->• $u-mm-la-mb(p)ika-na ->• 0u-mm-la-nja:-na the intrusive element in the above examples is a semivowel. it is not necessarily the velar semivowel though the result of the modification would appear to be the same (in several languages) for both w/y. • (h) 2q(c+cv) -*• (ccv) these are perhaps the most interesting and most widespread and best-known of the sound changes in bantu languages. they include 'nasalisation' (classes 9/10) and the many interesting effects arising from the loss of the class 5 prefix though this prefix may often be reinstated together with its effects. the presence of these changes means that classes 9/10 and class 5 virtually have their own greatly restricted phonology as indicated elsewhere in this paper. nasalisation in classes 9/10 the term 'nasalisation' represents a great variety of changes in not all of which there is invariably a nasal. these changes may be found with classes 9/10, with the reflexive (in sotho), with the object concord (in sotho, pedi, etc.), and in isolated cases elsewhere. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 e.o.j. westphal the prefix is hypostatised as ν which, for the southern bantu languages certainly, is a velar nasal with a possible latent vowel element. it is the velar aspect together with the vowel that is the basis of the formations which include the reflexive with no nasal element in sotho perhaps in the following starred sb** forms: sb** n+cv 1q(ncv) for cl. 9/10 nn+cv -» 2q(nn+ncv) for the object concord in sotho g¥i+cv -» 2q(gi+¥cv) for the reflexive infix in sotho the details are too numerous to exemplify and the following is no more than a comparative survey to mark the different kinds of stages of southern bantu 'nasalisation' illustrated from the labial only: n + cv-* 1q(ncv) yei: n+p->mp 1 q(n+effect+cv) nkhumbi: n + p ^ mhp 1 q(nc+effect+v) chewa: n+p -> mph xhosa: n+p^-mp' lq(nc+effect+v) sotho: n+p tfph lq(n<2+effect+v) shona: n+tf^mtfh 1 q(nc+effect+v) herero: n+p -> mb (also n+b -> mb) lqqjc+effect+v) sotho: n+b -» rip' effects of class 5 prefixal elements the effects that require description here are of two kinds viz. (1) a glottalisation process (implosion and voicing) in shona and venda and (2) a yotization process that will differentially summarise the shifts to alveolars and to labials (where *y is involved) in venda and in sotho. the shifts of the consonants can very incompletely be summarised as follows: (1) shona ¥ + ρ b t ->d k ->-g (implosive, voice) (voice) (2) shona ¥+ p -*· b t -*• d k -»-g venda ¥+ p -*· b t -»• d k -»-g venda ί + φ r h φη^· fu ru -*• pfhu hu fu β ν 1 dz φ -*• dz 0u->vu lu dzu 0u->· sotho i + { •* tshw r -+s,f h s b -> ts 1 -*• ts φ the processes are experimentally stated as follows: qp + q' (qp) + q" where qp is the prefix syllable and q" is the modified form of q' qp(c£i) + q(cv) -> ( qp(0) + q(scv) where € is the consonantal pro, cess and where the qp or prefix ( qp(lyi) syllable shows three venda and ( qp(li) sotho forms ea -> (¥ci ' (i=ci and u c i / . . . '+labialised vowel [u]' vci -*• c¥i ici cii and cui journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) syllable and sound change in southern bantu languages 41 from these latter forms it will now become evident why the results of the consonantal modifications are so similar to those of *cyv in these two languages. examples of these shifts include such as the following: (1) venda shambo/ marambo 'bone', shubi/marubi 'ruin', fumi/mahumi 'ten', shada/mahada 'shoulder', dzuvha 'pumpkin flower' cf. maluvha 'flower', vele 'seedgrain' cf. mavhele 'grain, maize', luvhele 'millet', vudzi/mavhudzi 'hair' (2) sotho: lesapo — lerapo 'bone', lesome 'ten' letsopa 'clay' cf. ve. vumba, letsoho_ 'arm', letshwafu 'lung'. in lovhedu, dogwa, pulana, and pedi and in birwa many further changes will be found. the centralising (alveolarising, palatalising) and frontalising (labialising) effects of this prefix are ascribed to the elements and **u and for this reason the effects of the process resemble those of **y and **w (in passives occasionally, and diminutives frequently). references 1. brown, g. (1970): syllables and redundancy rules./, of linguistics, 6, no. 1. 2. chafe, wallace l. (1970): meaning and the structure of language. university of chicago press. 3. chomsky, n., and halle, m. (1968): the sourid pattern of english. harper and row, new york. 4. fudge, e.c. (1973): on the notion 'universal phonetic framework'. in phonoledsi, ed., fudge, e.c. penguin books. 5. palmer, f.r. (1973): prosodic analysis. in phono led31, ed., fudge, e.c. penguin books. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c e r e b r a l p a l s y and t h e b o b a t h t r e a t m e n t . b y w.m. guldenpfennig. b.sc. (med.), m.b., ch.b., m.med.(neur.) 1 diagnosis, severe cases of cerebral palsy, presenting with pronounced abnormalities, are only too common. a palsy from cerebral causes can, however, be very mild presenting mainly as clumsiness or lack of full motor development, with or without mental retardation. in such cases precise diagnosis can be difficult and various conditions have to be considered. cerebellar defects present as clumsiness. some children are slow to walk and to speak and slow to learn complex motor activities due to mental deficiency. a somewhat similar condition is occasionally encountered in children of normal mentality with no signs of cerebral palsy to this is given the name "congenital maladroitness". the diagnosis of cerebral palsy is a clinical and not a pathological one. various types of cerebral palsy, e.g. diplegia, hemiplegia, choreoathetosis etc., are all clinical concepts each of which can be seen with various neuropathological conditions. accurate clinical diagnosis and assessment, together with response to treatment and the end result of such treatment, is important; not only from a purely clinical point of view, but also with a view to correlation with neuro-pathology and as a basis for further research work and development of newer therapeutic methods. from such careful clinical appraisal, the bobarth treatment for example developed. ί on the pathological side, brain biopsies are more often undertaken nowadays and gradually information is being accumulated which may later aid in morej accurate diagnosis and one hopes, in more specific treatments. an interesting newer field, is the study of abnormalities in chromosomes or chromosomal patterns. this work is also being done in pretoria and a project for the future is the study of the chromosomes in cerebral palsy. it is already well known that conditions such as mongolism and the klinefelter syndrome, are most probably due to an abnormal number of chromosomes. 2. physiology. in the second place, i wish to mention briefly some of the neuro-physiological factors involved in cerebral palsy. the accent in cerfebral palsy is apt to be on the motor abnormality, and with this in mind, we are often inclined to split up motor function into so-called component parts and even to speak of higher and lower levels of motor function, forgetting sometimes that normal motor function is one perfectly integrated whole in which no level is higher or lower in importance or effect, as every part of the motor system is necessary for normal function, each playing its part as required. furthermore no motor system can function efficiently without a sensory input and the sensory input may be regarded as the most important factor in motor function, whether this be in the spinal cord or in the physical sphere. for purposes of description, however, we must divide up the nervous system and speak of various functional systems. (a) cortical. we all know the importance of the sensorimotor area of the cortex in discreet actions or movements. the exact functions of the various cortical layers are still not known and although a great significance has been attached to the large betz cells in the fifth layer as origin of the so-called pyramidal tract, we know today that these cells alone cannot account for all pyramidal fibres and functions. many other cells in the sensori-motor area take part in the formation of the pyramidal tract, and these different types of classes of fibres from the cortex, have slightly different functions. two of these, for 11 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) example, are the alpha motor pathways and gamma motor pathways from the cortex supplying impulses to the alpha and gamma motor efferents to muscles via the anterior horn of the spinal cord. the activity of these systems depends on cortical and subcortical activity. it was found that the cortex as a whole, not only possesses a potential excitability, but that it is probably at most times in a varying state of inhibition. this cortical inhibition may be altered by various routes, one of which, e.g. is thought to function as a feed-back system from the cortex, through the basal ganglia and thalamus back to the cortex by which means .the state of cortical inhibition can be increased or diminished, depending most probably on the rate of discharge by the feed-back circuit. this feed-back circuit is in its turn influenced by other circuits from the basal ganglia, the reticular formation, the cerebellum, the vestibular apparatus, and the spinal nerves and the muscles. an example of extreme cortical inhibition is to be found in many cases of post-epileptic paralyses, where the inhibitory effect may be so strong as to cause temporary complete paralysis of a limb or limbs. the importance of the state of cortical inhibition lies in its effect on muscle tone as well as its effect on phasic contractions. (b) reticular system. one of the most important mechanisms in motor effect is the reticular system of the brainstem, which by its. wide spread afferent connections and its diffuse projections to the cortex via the thalamus, plays an extensive role in all motor functions. cerebellar effects on motor function are in part exerted through fibres from the purkinje cells which pass to the reticular substance. the frequency of impulse formation is thought to cause either facilitation or inhibition. probably slow rhythmical impulses cause inhibition. when cortical inhibition is diminished, more alpha motor neurone firing occurs, so that the balance balance between alpha and gamma effect is disturbed and increase of muscle tone then results. such conditions may occur in parkinsonism, e.g. where due to loss of function of the substantia nigra, less modulating effect is exerted on the feed-back circuit, increased speed of impulse formation passes through the thalamus, decrease in cortical inhibition and therefore, relative increase in alpha motor effect occurs with resultant increase in muscle tone. i may add here that the substantia nigra, nucleus ruber and corpus subthalamicum are regarded by some merely as specially developed parts of the reticular system. not only does this system have its effect on the basal ganglia, thalamus and cortex, but it also has a more direct effect on spinal functions and especially on muscle tone. its effect is most probably in the region of the internuncial neurone rather than on the anterior horn cell itself. the vestibular influences on spinal functions are to a large extent dependant on the close connections with the reticular system. (c) spinal cord. in the spinal cord we find the continuation ofthe reticular system, where again it plays an important part in facilitation and inhibition. in traumatic paraplegia for example, it is thought that due to the local abnormalities, artificial synapses are formed, so that afferent impulses in the region of the injury are easily transmitted causing reticular facilitation and therefore, resulting in spasticity. because of the inhibition or facilitation that can probably originate in the cord itself, a second transection of the spinal cord can increase spasticity, by increasing local facilitation. (d) muscles. we have recently read more and more about the effect of the gamma system on muscle tone. this gamma system depends on socalled muscle spindles in between and parallel to the other muscle fibres, which function as indicators of change of muscle tension. through sensory afferents from these spindles, different degrees of facilitation of the anterior horn cells and the alpha motor efferents are effected, causing change in muscle tone. these muscle spindles are in their turn supplied by special gamma efferent fibres by means of 12 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) which their own tension and therefore sensitivity, can be altered. s u m m a r y . from, this short discussion on some aspects of neuro-physiological function, one realises that the nature of normal function is still very obscure and that abnormalities are therefore very difficult to explain. it is, however, the abnormalities that necessitate treatment, and if is from clinical observation of abnormal function and the manner in which normal and abnormal function can be influenced, that the bobarth treatment was evolved. opsomming. uit hierdie kort bespreking oor 'n paar aspekte van neurofisiologiese funksies, is dit diiidelik dat die aard van normale funksies nog steeds baie onbekend is en daarom is abnormale funksies baie moeilik om te verklaar. dit is egter vir die abnormaliteite wat vehandeling nodig is en uit kliniese waarneming van die abnormale funksies en die manier waarop normale en abnormale funksies beinvloed word, het die bobathbehandelingsmetodes ontstaan. 13 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 35 the mind-mapping approach (mma): a culture and language "free" technique caroline m. leaf isobel c. uys brenda louw department of speech pathology and audiology university of pretoria abstract although the roots of the mind-mapping approach (mma) reach back into the depths of psychology, it is our growing understanding of the human brain, how it functions, what affects it, how we can assist it which has become the real foundation for the model discussed in this paper. by finding ways of creating environments that are brain compatible or rather brain enhancing, we can begin to serve the whole person in all his dimensions. in this paper the need for language and culture "free" therapeutic techniques will be discussed. reference is made to the author's research where the mma was used very successfully with a closed head injured (chi) client in order to demonstrate the effectiveness of the technique. the problem of chi is also discussed. in addition, a brief theoretical review of the brain as it pertains to the concept of the mma is offered. in the conclusion, it is suggested that global techniques such as the mma, which are based on fundamental and universal principles, are the route to finding language and culture "free" techniques. alhoewel die oorsprong van die breinkaartbenadering tot die grondbeginsels van die sielkunde teruggevoer kan word, is dit ons toenemende begrip van die menslike brein, die funksionering daarvan, waardeur dit beinvloed word en hoe breinfunksionering ondersteun kan word, wat die werklike grondslag uitmaak van die model wat in hierdie artikel bespreek word. deur wyses te vind vir die daarstelling van omgewings wat breinfunksionering versterk, kan η vertrekpunt gevind word vanwaar ons die klient as geheel in al sy dimensies kan benader. die behoefte aan "kultuurvrye" terapeutiese tegnieke word bespreek. daar word verwys na die outeur se navorsmg waar die breinkaart as terppiemodel met sukses aangewend is by 'n geslotehoofbeseerde klient. die problematiek van geslote hoofbeserings word ook bespreek. daarbenewens word 'n kort teoretiese oorsig van die brem, met betrekkmg tot die konsep van die breinkaartbenadering tot intervensie, verskaf. ten slotte word daar voorgestel dat globale tegnieke soos die breinkaartbenadering, wat gebaseer is op fundamentele en universele beginsels, die weg is waarvolgens "kultuurvrye" tegnieke gevind sal word. opsomming die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 introduction speech-language and hearing therapy can be defined as "the delivery of services to (all) individuals with speechlanguage-hearing disorders, aimed at the amelioration of communication difficulties stemming from such disorders" (uys & hay, 1985). therefore, the provision of quality services to all groups and individuals is the goal of speech and language therapists worldwide. however, socio-economic, sociopolitical and demographic shifts over time have intensified "existing inequities in service delivery to specific groups" (asha, 1991:5). in fact a great deal of disparity in service delivery exists in the profession (asha, 1991). taylor (1986, ρ 3), attributes this disparity to two factors viz: speech therapists have a poor perception of the distinction "between a legitimate linguistic difference and a speech and language disorder". secondly all communication norms are based on a middle-class euro-american model (taylor, 1986). clearly, there is a need for greater understanding of dialect and language differences and for a clinical perspective that appropriately addresses these differences. in south africa it is extremely difficult to meet the needs of all individuals and to fulfil these goals as speech therapists are faced with two unique problems, namely: the therapist-patient ratio which is approximately 1 : 6000 annually; and the widely spread, multicultural, multilingual society (uys & hay, 1985). therefore, in order to be effective, a therapist needs to learn a minimum of 30 languages and as many cultures (uys & hay, 1985). based on this argument, a therapist is not able to give effective accountable therapy cross-culturally. hence, the need arises for culture and language free techniques that deal with this problem. in this article, the mind-mapping approach (mma) developed by leaf (1990) for closed head injured (chi) patients, will be discussed as a possible language and culture "free" t e c h n i q u e for the m u l t i l i n g u a l and multicultural population group specifically of south africa. the authors view language and culture as interdependent concepts, thus the language used is a reflection of the culture. the mma is a metacognitive technique and therefore the linguistic elements are not worked on directly. thus, language is used as a tool in the mma, as the mma is a metacognitive technique (leaf, 1990). if linguistic delays are presented by the client, these can be treated within the framework of the mma. mind-mapping : a therapeutic technique for chi. chi presents speech and language therapists with a unique diagnostic, prognostic and treatment challenge as a result of the widespread diffuse damage experienced by these patients, as well as the tendency to a lengthy recovery (hagen, 1984). the general lack of understanding of the sequelae of chi has inhibited the development of assessment tools, and few treatment guidelines have been proposed in literature. thus there is a need for clinicians working with head injured patients to be able to identify and develop treatment regimes, that have functional goals caroline m. leaf, isobel c. uys, brenda louw involving behaviours that will make a difference in the way each person functions in his or her cultural environment (adamovich, 1986). due to the need for the development of accountable therapy techniques that deal with the complex problems of chi, the strategy of mind-mapping therapy was developed and tested on a chi subject (leaf, 1990). as this research yielded exciting results, the technique will be elucidated in order to demonstrate the flexibility and effectiveness of this approach. it also provides a theoretical underpinning for the proposed concept of the mma as a culture and language "free" technique. the mind-mapping approach (mma) can be defined as a technique that organises and taps information in the brain by stimulating a synergistic effect between both hemispheres (leaf, 1990). it is a way of structuring information that assists in the learning of new information (leaf, 1990). as it is a psychoneurogenic technique based on the complex functioning of the brain, a brief theoretical review on the functioning of the brain as it pertains to the concept of the mma is necessary. the relationship between the functioning of the brain and the mind-mapping approach (mma). it is an often quoted statistic that humans use only 10% of their potential brain power. however, the more researchers have learnt in the last 10 years, the less likely they are to attempt to quantify brain potential. the only apparent consistent conclusion is that the proportion of our potential brain power that is used is probably nearer 0.1% than 10% (russell, 1986). in order to more fully utilize the brain's capacity one needs to consider each of the elements that comprise the way the brain functions. once one begins to understand how the brain operates the way is opened to tap that vast unused potential. in this regard sperry (1982), the pioneer of the first 'split-brain' experiments, that is cerebral localization studies on the functions of the two hemispheres, indicates that the integration of the left (more analytical) hemisphere and right (more creative) hemisphere, leads to enhanced functioning whilst thinking and learning. ! the general opinion in the available literature is that the two hemispheres process information differently (dual brain theory). the intensity of the involvement of each depends upon the specific task (sperry, 1982). for example, the hemispheres do not recall in sentences but in key / concepts and images (ornstein, 1975). of the words heard, / spoken or seen, only 1% to 10% are essential key words (buzan, 1991). every word is multi-ordinate, i.e. it has a large number of links to other words (buzan, 1991). in addition to this multi-ordinate nature of words, each brain is unique in the way it perceives images and is by nature both creative and organising (zaidel, 1985). finally, because recall is in images and words as opposed to sentences, thought processes are multi-dimensional and nonlinear (buzan, 1991). , ornstein indicates that by cultivating both hemispheres, the brain returns to a natural holistic way of functioning which in turn unlocks potential. thus, stimulating a synergy between the two hemispheres is critical to effective accelerated learning. hagin (in van kraayenberg, 1992), has pioneered research using techniques that stimulate brain hemisphere synergy on severely learning disabled children with remarkable success. some of the techthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the mind-mapping approach (mma): a culture and language "free" technique 37 niques he has researched are ambidextrous writing and numerical techniques. an additional example of this important research is research in the field of cerebral functioning, that has been conducted over the last 15 20 years, namely the attempt to ascertain the biological processes involved in learning and memory (rosenzweig & bennet, 1976). it is well documented that the brain is adaptable and that the brain appears to compensate for the damaged nerve fibres by: rerouting to alternative areas, the development of innate potentials in other areas of the brain, and gradual recovery from shock caused by injury (luria, 1982). a fact that has also emerged from this research and that is very useful in terms of an evolving philosophy of remediation is that these experiments have seemingly confirmed the susceptibility of the brain to actual physical and clinical changes resulting from training or enrichment of the environment (rosenzweig & bennet, 1976). the possibility therefore exists that, under the right conditions, neurons may regenerate themselves, and be capable of growing new fibres indicating that they do have the genetic potential for reproduction. it has not yet been ascertained whether or not such a regeneration can occur in a normal nervous system, but the brain appears to make the maximum use of the possibilities open to it, and one of the most likely places to find such changes in nerve cell tissues would be in damaged tissue (russell, 1986). rosenzweig and bennet (1976) indicate that much of the research done in this area will undoubtedly lead to applications such as the alleviation of learning disability caused by neural malfunctions in perception, the prevention of senile decline in memory and learning, and better means of packaging information to aid learning and retrieval. this is the principle on which mind-mapping therapy (leaf, 1990) is based. of particular interest to the above concepts is the research by scott (1976), concerning neuron regeneration. scott (1976) found that neurons can be induced to reproduce themselves if placed in a medium enriched in potassium ions which are veryj important to axonal electrical conduction this is called the k-mitotic effect. scott (1976) argued that human adult neurons have the genetic potential to reproduce themselves. unfortunately, however, the intriguing question is still left open as to why these neurons cannot exploit this reaction so as to produce true regeneration. scott's findings, do however, make some inroads into the traditional idea that the neurons of the central nervous system are incapable of regeneration. in evaluating the results of applying the mind-mapping strategy to a chi patient (leaf, 1990), spontaneous recovery cannot be overlooked as it is a controversial issue in terms of its interaction with treatment. it is estimated, that spontaneous recovery in a chi subject extends up to 18 months post-accident (adamovich, 1986). an additional complicating factor is that uncertainty exists about how treatment interacts with spontaneous recovery (adamovich, 1986). based on the foregoing discussion on brain functioning, it is possible that mind-mapping therapy encourages spontaneous recovery to extend beyond the spontaneous capacity of the brain for physiological and structural change. therefore, the mma is a neuropsychological metacognitive-communication intervention technique that attempts to utilise to the fullest possible extent, the functional plasticity of neural tissue (leaf, 1990). the concept is based on the idea that if the brain is given suitable and varied stimulation, it will continue developing and growing through life (rosenzweig & bennet, 1976). it is hypothesized that when mind-mapping therapy is initiated, luria's (1982), process of intersystematic and intrasystematic reorganisation occurs. as the environment becomes enriched and progressively more organised as a result of this stimulation, new unused areas (previously not stimulated) take over function. as a result of this, an increase in potassium and sodium at the synapses probably occurs. these are important neurochemicals that can encourage collateral sprouting and regeneration of damaged neurons (scott, 1976; russell, 1986). the latter point has only been proved in the central nervous system (cns) of animals (scott, 1976) and, as most of our neuropsychological knowledge is based on application of animal research to humans, the above is a plausible deduction (leaf, 1990). therefore, it is hypothesized that humans have a preset template of cognitive functioning that is moulded by the environment and mediators (significant people in the environment). in the brain's functional systems, however, there are other stores, i.e. potential connections that if stimulated, can carry out the cognitive activity lost or disrupted. the rationale for this is the unlimited potential of the brain and our underuse of it, that is we limit our brain by only using certain functional systems, whilst others lie dormant. if disruption due to brain damage occurs, the brain tries to restore equilibrium via compensatory strategies, for example, the patient elaborates, circumlocutes and perseverates (penn and cleary, 1988). in the mma, the therapist acts as a mediator, and through a process of stimulating both the right and left hemispheres, the patient is assisted in searching the subconscious, unused stores to urdock this dormant potential and/or stimulate the regeneration of neurons (known as the k-mitotic effect). ylvisaker and szekeres (1986) also feel that increased spontaneous recovery can be effected by providing an enriched environment. according to leaf (1990), this concept advocates a plasticity of the brain with a reorganisation emphasis, as opposed to plasticity of the brain with a change in localisation of function emphasis. therefore, with the mma it is advocated that a new area of the brain is stimulated to learn to perform the function and, in doing this, the possibility that an enriched environment enabling regeneration and therefore restoration of the damaged functional system occurs. this new concept stimulating brain hemisphere synergy, implies learning as opposed to equipotentiality and substitution, which implies a pre-existing spread of any function over the area this underestimates the potential of the brain (powell, 1981). there are many uncharted areas in the brain, and we are only beginning to understand our abilities. for instance anokhin (1985 in russell, 1986) calculated that the brain is capable of making 1 followed by 10 million kilometres of typewritten 0's of connections. furthermore, he estimated that an area the size of a pea in the human brain could control the entire world's telephone network system. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 caroline . leaf, isobel c. uys, brenda louw development of a conceptual framework as a rationale for developing the mind-mapping approach (mma). the concepts of brain-behaviour relationships, cognition and information processing will be discussed in the following section, in order to elucidate the rationale of the mind-mapping approach (mma) developed by the first author and used with a chi subject (leaf, 1990). brain-behaviour relationships luria's emphasis on understanding brain functions as interlocking functional subsystems has been a particularly important concept in the mind-mapping approach (luria, 1982). while the diffuse and complex neuropsychological deficits that seem to accompany severe traumatic head injury make it difficult to apply lurian assessment and remediation techniques, his general approach to appreciating the interconnectedness of many cognitive functions has been vital. according to luria (1982), the destruction of cortical tissue has an effect on the entire functional system to which it contributes because a link within the functional chain has been weakened or damaged. two situations can occur, namely:intersystematic incorporating a new link from a different functional system; intrasystematic vertical shift within a functional system (luria, 1982). in the mma it is hypothesized that, initially both intersystemic and intrasystemic reorganisation occur (leaf, 1990). this could lead to the areas around the neurons becoming enriched because of this stimulation resulting in two possible occurrences, namely:new unused areas take over function; and collateral sprouting and regeneration (scott, 1976) occur resulting in restoration of function. restoration to original and possibly higher levels of original functioning may occur. l b conclude: by applying lurian concepts mind-mapping therapy can be better understood. in the next section mind-mapping therapy will be viewed from the cognitive perspective in order to further elucidate this concept. the authors feel that a paradigm shift as to how we view our mental capacities, and those of our clients, is essential if we are to be more effective in our changing south africa. cognition. from the perspective of the information processing model, cognition involves a complex process with which an individual processes information for particular purposes, within certain mental structures and environmental constraints (ylvisaker & szekeres, 1986). cognition is viewed by feuerstein (1980) as mental acts that enable a person to think and therefore act intelligently. he adds another dimension to his definition by stating that well developed cognitive functions are the product of mediated learning experiences, i.e. the way in which stimuli emitted by the environment are transformed by a "mediating" agent, usually a parent, sibling or other caregiver. through this process of mediation the cognitive structure of the child is affected. the child acquires behaviour patterns and learning sets, which in turn become important ingredients of his capacity to become modified through direct exposure to stimuli, hence cultural differences. since direct exposure to stimuli quantitatively constitutes the greatest source of the organism's exposure, the existence of sets of strategies and repertoires that permit the organism to efficiently use this exposure has considerable bearing upon cognitive development. feuerstein's objective therefore, is "to change the cognitive structure of the retarded performer and to transform him into an autonomous independent thinker capable of initiating and elaborating ideas" (hobbs in feuerstein, 1980:viii). this concept of modifiable cognition is what the first author's research aimed to investigate, thus feuerstein's orientation is considered a further critical aspect of mma theory (leaf, 1990). feuerstein (1980) is interested in how the organism learns and solves problems. in mma therapy, the thought processes of the brain are being stimulated as information is being structured according to the way the brain functions. thus, deficient thought processes and learning skills can be identified as the mind-maps are being constructed because the clients' thought processes are being evaluated. at the same time, correct patterns can be set up, which would be therapy. feuerstein's (1980) interest in the formal structure of thought as opposed to the content of the mind, has relevance in that, with the mma which, aims at improving information processing, and cognitive processes, the formal structure of thought is also being improved. l b summarise, the mma, like the feuerstein approach, becomes a teach-test-teach paradigm and the therapist tries to promote the best possible learning and motivational conditions. the mma programme progresses through simple to complex reasoning tasks. it is dynamic because the person's learning style is assessed in the act of learning which highlights the metacognitive strategies and styles that the person is using. the technique also encourages the individual to take charge thus ensuring carryover. this in turn, guides specific therapeutic intervention (leaf, 1990). in the following section the mma is further evaluated in terms of information processing. information processing. i information processing refers to the analysis and synthesis of information in sequential steps (neisser, 197,6 in ylvisaker, 1986). information processing abilities can be divided into three stages, namely:j regulation of input j encoding storage retrieval i regulation of response (ylvisaker & szekeres, 1986).1 these stages become the goals of developing information processing skills. in information processing terms, humans are seen as highly sophisticated computers with elaborate programmes. viewed from this perspective/the mma can be seen as the operating system somewhat comparable to ms dos (the operating system of a computer) of the brain allowing the programmes to work and, the more efficiently the operating system is,being used, the greater the functioning of the programme. the techniques, forms and manner of the mma. the term 'mma rehabilitation' is used in a very broad sense to indicate the treatment of the cognitive communicative deficits identified in patients, namely: cognitively the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the mind-mapping approach (mma): a culture andlanguage "free" technique 39 based communicative, behavioural and psychosocial deficits, as well as the narrowly defined deficits in component systems and processes (ylvisaker & szekeres, 1986). since the scope of the mma is broad, an attempt is made to make it more manageable by distinguishing the general principles, forms and manner of the approach that guides treatment decisions. the general principles provide the environment with therapy: the form of intervention provides the philosophy: and finally, the manner of intervention provides the way information in therapy is analysed. the mind-mapping technique described in this article was evolved into a specific treatment approach for chi by the first author based on a literature review, and research. thus the mind-mapping protocol is original research and is supported by a strong theoretical base (leaf, 1990). the techniques of the mma rehabilitation. the overall objective of the mma is to achieve the effective functioning of patients in normal situations comparable to premorbid functioning. the general techniques of the mma are comparable to those of any treatment regime. there are a few specfic techniques, however, that are unique to the mind-mapping programme. these include techniques that are not uncommon in the realm of therapy such as relaxation, breathing and music. it is the combination of techniques that the researcher has put together as part of the standard procedure of mind-mapping, that is unique. the first of these techniques is relaxation. relaxation means, "a realistic response to the environment with a minimum of needless energy expended" (boone, 1977, ρ 151). therefore, by using relaxation techniques, unnecessary tension is reduced. on a physiological level, relaxation results in an increase in secretion of the brain's natural opiates endorphin and enkephalin which are generated when one feels good or relaxed (hand, 1986). this in turn relaxes the limbic system's negative potential, enabling the neurochemicals necessary for learning and remembering to be generated (hand, 1986). thus intellectual pursuit is allowed to progress more readily than when negative feelings prevail, and! the patient is guided towards comfort management rather than stress management. according to wolpe (1958) |"as we inhibit the anxiety through relaxation, the stuttering problem recedes" (boone, 1977). j the second technique of the mma is the use of controlled breathing. boone (1977) suggests the use of breathing techniques for voice patients with poor breathing patterns. russell (1986), indicates that breathing exercises aid concentration by focusing attention on the energy interplay involved in breathing. according to hand (1986), even though our brain is only approximately 3.5 pounds, it requires 25% of the body's oxygen intake to function optimally. for these reasons, breathing exercises are incorporated within the mma approach. the third technique incorporated into the mma is the use of suggestion and visualisation. according to van riper (in boone, 1977), suggestion and visualisation permeate all forms of therapy, including those practised in medicine. no patient seeks help without some expectation that his/her problem will be alleviated. therefore, directly or indirectly, deliberately or unconsciously, some kind of suggestion and visualisation seems to be inherent in any therapeutic relationship. according to lozanov & gateva (1989), using suggestion and visualisation enables people to make more effective use of their brain potential, because suggestion has a similar effect to relaxation physiologically (discussed under the principle of relaxation), and visualisation is essential to the process of activating memory (russell, 1986). fourthly, the use of music specifically baroque music is advocated by the mma to be used throughout therapy. music is used due to the documented effect it has on the alpha and beta wave forms of the brain (lozanov & gateva, 1989). music activates neurons for purposes of relaxing muscle tension, changing pulse and producing long-range memories which are directly related to the number of neurons activated in the experience (hand, 1986). music relaxes major portions of the brain so that those which are active encounter little interference from other portions. furthermore, there are different types of music for different types of activities, that is learning, relaxation and visualisation (lozanov, & gateva, 1989). baroque music specifically is important as this type of music is characterized as being generally relaxing, with a 4/4 rhythm and 60 beats per minute. the relaxation is induced due to psychological entrainment of body rhythms (hand, 1986). therefore, the techniques of relaxation, breathing, suggestion and visualisation, and music create the environment of the mma procedure, the main objective being to create an optimal learning situation. finally, there are various other techniques derived from brain-behaviour relationships that are applied to the mma intervention. these principles are adapted from hand (1986) and luria (1982), and include the following: words are read aloud this activates broca's area, wernicke's area, left hemisphere sensory and motor cortex and the angular gyrus. ' words are read with emotion and inflection this activates right hemisphere areas for prosodic functions, right motor and sensory cortex, and the limbic system. pictorial images are developed and mind-maps are used throughout thus the patient is being stimulated to think in a visual and organised way. this involves a major portion of the primary visual cortex, left and right motor sensory cortices, central and peripheral nervous system from the brain stem through the arms, hands and fingers. key words on mind-maps allow for discrimination in the right visual cortex. testing after review this reinforces neuronal connections established during initial learning, leading to hypertrophy and/or branching of neuron dendrites, and making recall easier. the more vivid and active the impression of what is being learned, the stronger the memory trace. the spike of electrical activity in the brain increases markedly with novel, surprising or vivid stimuli. this activity signals the hippocampus and hypothalamus to produce increased levels of neurochemicals related to memory formation. the forms of the mma. rehabilitation efforts in general may take the form of facilitating spontaneous recovery, direct retraining of cognitive components, retraining functional-integrative perdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 formance and compensation training. the ylvisaker & szekeres (1986) approach indicates each of these forms of intervention as separate approaches in rehabilitation through which the patient in general progresses through, terminating in compensation training to overcome deficits not remediated directly. the mind-mapping approach is more eclectic than the traditional therapy discussed above, and holistic in that the process of therapy is seen to work on components and functional-integrative performance concurrently. therefore, there is no distinction between component and functional-integrative performance (ylvisaker & szekeres, 1986). furthermore, it is felt that the mma encourages the brain to go beyond its capacity for spontaneous recovery, because patients are being taught a strategy that stimulates the whole brain resulting in increased efficiency (leaf, 1990). in conclusion the mma is not a form of compensation. in this approach, compensation is seen as for example, using logbooks to assist memory, or wearing glasses. mindmapping therapy is more, it is a strategy to stimulate whole-brain learning. the manner of mma: textlinguistics/metaphors and the creation of the mind-map. the analysis of textlinguistics as a vehicle for examining discourse beyond the sentence level has recently received attention by various groups of researchers (liles, 1985). textlinguistics provides a logical ordered way of approaching the content (story, newspaper article, schoolwork) of therapy. therefore, the story (metaphor) or macrostructure model of analysis provides the manner in which the mma is done, that is, the manner in which the texts oral and written used in therapy are approached. textlinguistics also provides the organized steps of the ' thinking process that goes into the actual creation of the mind-map. thus, the mind map becomes the visual representation of the cognitive process. the actual creation of the mind map stimulates both the left and right hemispheres, which increases the effectiveness of functioning. therefore, a mutually beneficial cyclic process is set up (buzan, 1991). based on the conceptual framework as discussed in 2.1. and the principles, forms and manner of the mma in 2.2., a treatment model was designed by the first author. this is outlined in the form of a schematic layout in figure 1. in the treatment model, therapy is viewed as being divided into two aspects, namely the creation of an optimal learning environment, and then the actual process/steps of therapy. in the first, the brain is being prepared' or grimed' to receive information and to function optimely by ensuring the correct physiological aspects (electrical and chemical and oxygen). the process takes the client through various steps which stimulate brain hemisphere synergy. discussion of the application of the mma to a single case study of chi. a number of questions were addressed in a study by leaf (1990) regarding the effectiveness of mind-mapping as a therapeutic intervention technique on a white female subject. the results revealed a statistically significant improvement in the academic ability of the chi subject, caroline m. leaf, isobel c. uys, brenda louw which was proved to be directly attributable to the intervention program. there was also a statistically significant improvement of the subject in the indirectly treated cognitive-language skills and the untreated pragmatic skills, indicating generalization effects. it is this finding that supports the postulation that the mma is a language and culture "free" technique. the results furthermore revealed increasingly competent post-treatment performance academically. with specific regard to cognitive language abilities, it was deduced that mind-mapping improved the following skills in the subject (leaf, 1990): to interpret alternative meanings in ambiguous statements by evaluating multiple meanings, features at the surface structure and features at the deep structure level to reason analytically, synthetically and logically to interpret abstract concepts to improve retrieval so that it becomes an organised efficient memory search the processing of information to associate and identify relationships to select central concepts to identify, retrieve, sequence and produce semantic units expression to identify, recall and retrieve appropriate grammatical structures expression to make judgements about concepts to attend and perceive. music creation of optimum learning environment 'understand concepts of mind-mapping and brain potential 'key concept identification * understand mind-mapping approach, namely overview, preview, invlew, learning & memory, and review metaphor: application of mlnd*mapplng approach to stories application of mlnd*mapping approach to academic learning skills figure 1. schematic layout of mind-mapping intervention preparation breathing relaxation visualisation co ( / ) l u ο ο dc α . phase one phase two phase three phase four the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the mind-mapping approach (mma): a culture andlanguage ree" technique 41 as the above receptive and expressive skills in the subject of the study (leaf, 1990) were indirectly treated, the suggestion is that a certain amount of generalization took place. this implies that the cognitive dysfunction underlies the language function further supporting the concept of the mma being language and culture "free". the improved psychosocial and pragmatic functioning of the subject which were the untreated behaviours in the study (leaf, 1990) proved interesting as prigatano (1986) had the same results in his intensive neuropsychological rehabilitation programme, but he had used professional neuropsychologists and clinical psychologists to help the patients as a separate part of their treatment. in the study conducted by leaf (1990), the only therapy received by the subject was mind-mapping therapy conducted by a speech and language therapist, however, the same type of effects were obtained. conclusion: application of the mma cross-culturally. in order to overcome the linguistic and cultural diversity that exists the world over, the study of normal and pathological communication must be couched in cultural terms. this would avoid the risk of making judgements about the communication of a given group from an inappropriate set of assumptions and norms (taylor, 1986). furthermore, cultural factors also determine the definition of what is pathological, i.e. the definition of the quality and normalcy of the environment (taylor, 1986). likewise, "treatment should take into account the preferred learning style of the client and the rules of social and communicative interaction as defined by the client's indigeneous cultural or linguistic group" (taylor, 1986, ρ 7). therefore all clinical encounters are cultural events requiring an ethnological approach. this implies a need for greater understanding of dialectical and language differences. j "however, the adoption of a dialectical perspective need not imply dramatically different clinical methods for every dialect" (taylor, 1986). that is, the universal principles of therapy would not alter, j but the material would. the learner is a product of his environment, thus, the material should be selected from his specific culture. in this way the learning process is concentrated on, and not the material. j current diagnostic and treatment procedures emphasize middle class westernized values and are therefore not "culture free" (bogatz in taylor, 1986). the therapist working in a multicultural environment needs to expand the assessment and treatment process, to determine the client's learning potential as opposed to acquired knowledge (taylor, 1986). mind-mapping, as discussed, is a way of structuring information according to the way the brain functions, stimulating both hemispheres to function in harmony (buzan, 1984). in this way, learning potential can be assessed and improved. the mma (leaf, 1990) is the application of this concept into the realms of therapy by providing the clients with a more efficient "operating system" allowing them to use/access their potential more efficiently. it appeals directly to the multi-dimensional cognitive level and therefore reflects the cognitive processes underlying language. mind-mapping therapy incorporates a unique combination of techniques that have been shown to stimulate a more efficient process of learning new information (leaf, 1990). these include relaxation, breathing, and music which provide the environment of therapy (see figure 1), which can be adapted to suit each individual. by using the mma (leaf, 1990), the patient is not simply observing long lists of words or sentences, rather he is receiving each word in the context of the words that surround it. at the same time he is also giving the multiordinate nature of each word his own special interpretation as dictated by the structure of his personal information patterns thus "culture free" and, will be analysing, coding and criticising throughout the process. in applying the mma to a single chi subject (leaf, 1990), only multi-dimensional cognitive skills as they pertained to academic abilities were worked on directly, showing a statistically significant improvement. however, the indirectly treated receptive and expressive language skills, and untreated pragmatic and psychosocial functioning also improved significantly, allowing the subject to return to her premorbid level of functioning. this stresses the universality of the mma, strengthening the possibility of the mma being a culture and language "free" technique. from this, one could postulate that by working via a common medium such as academic material, through using the mma, one could circumvent the problem of language and culture bias, l b this end, the mma lends itself to being used within a consultative framework, such as suggested by uys (1985), where a professional service is given indirectly through the use of a "consulting care person" (uys & hay 1985, ρ 4). in other words, the mma could be used within an empowerment framework, as the mma is easy and effective to use in group situations. in this way large sectors of the population could be reached. if a client is not schoolgoing, illiterate or has severely depressed functioning due to a neurogenic disorder, the mma still has applicability due to its multidimensional and sensory nature. it is also a highly visual technique, lending itself to images and pictures instead of words, if this is required. the first author has applied the technique on a clinical basis cross-culturally with blacks, coloureds, indians, chinese and japanese. the results have been encouraging in terms of linguistic, cognitive and psychosocial functioning. various trends were also observed where certain of the mma techniques were more appealing to certain cultures. for example, the black students learned material more effectively using music by steven halpern & savary (adapted baroque), whilst learning new activities. although this music has no identifiable tune and does not have the orthodox tempo of 60 beats/minute, it has been shown to facilitate whole-brain learning (halpern & savary, 1985). clynes (in halpern, 1985) indicates that there are specific forms of emotional expression called "essentic forms" that act like keys in a lock and activate specific learning memory processes. research suggests that essentic forms have innate meanings that transcend cultural learning and conditioning, and are therefore neurologically coded. it is possible that these essentic forms are more activated using this adapted type of music with black clients. critchley and henderson (in halpern & savary, 1985) found that this adapted baroque music may be able to die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 caroline . leaf, isobel c. uys, brenda louw activate the flow of stored memory material across the corpus callosum, so that right and left hemispheres of the brain work in harmony rather than in conflict, resulting in improved functioning. the asian students, on the other hand, preferred the standard combinations of classical and baroque music to the adapted music, learning more effectively with the former. lozanov & gateva (1989), using biofeedback, researched the effect of the standard classical and baroque music, finding it to be a catalyst during the process of instruction with students. the above clinical observations strengthen the premise that the technique of using music in mind-mapping therapy is a fundamental principle, and that the material type of music has a culture preference. futher corroborating the strength of the mma as an alternative fundamental "culture-free" technique, is research conducted by van kraayenburg (1992). he applied a similar combination of techniques to that of mind-mapping therapy, within a scholastic programme to teach english to 540 black south african schoolchildren in sub b, std 1 and std 2. the result was that the programme was judged foremost by the human sciences research council, after two and a half years of evaluation against seven other suggested teaching programmes. in conclusion, not only do we experience heterogeneity cross-culturally, but within each culture group there is diversity as well. using more global techniques such as mind-mapping therapy, where the individual's perception, learning style and potential to learn and think are emphasized as opposed to a group or culture's language and thought patterns, bias can be circumvented. thus, the aim should be to develop more global approaches to therapy focusing on creating richly varied culturally appropriate instructional environments. this should be done in a climate that fosters imagination, adventure and risk taking, all vital catalysts to learning. in this way we as speechlanguage therapists can try to deliver services to all individuals. einstein once said that imagination is more important than knowledge as knowledge is limited and imagination is not (buzan, 1990). the authors believe that one of the ways to deal with the mammoth task of delivering services to all cultures in south africa is to go beyond the realms of knowledge into that of imagination. hopefully then, culture and language "free" techniques will be found. references. adamovich, b.b. (1986). speciality recognition in neurogenic speech, language and cognitive disorders: training needs of speech-language pathologists regarding the cognitive rehabilitation of closed head injured persons. in r. brookshire (ed.), clinical aphasiology conference proceedings (ac). 16, 329-337. minneapolis: brk publishers. asha. (1991). reach: a model for the service delivery and professional development within remote/rural regions of the united states and u.s. territories. asha, 33, 6, 5-14. buzan, t. (1991). use both sides of your brain. united states: first plume printing. boone, d.r. (1977). the voice and voice therapy. new jersey: prentice-hall. feuerstein, r. (1980). instrumental enrichment: an intervention programme for cognitive modifiability. baltimore: university park press. hagen, c. (1984). language disorders in head trauma. in a.l. holland (ed.), language disorders.in adults: recent advances. san diego: college-hill press. halpern, s and savary, l. (1985). sound health: the music and sounds that make us whole. san francisco: harper & row publishers. hand, j.d. (1986). the brain and accelerative learning. per linguam, 2, 2, 214. hay, i.s. & uys, i.c. (1985) taalverskeidenheid en taalpatalogie: universiteit van pretoria. leaf, c.m. (1990). mind-mapping: a therapeutic technique for closed head injury. unpublished master's dissertation, university of pretoria. liles, b. (1985). production and comprehension of narrative discourse in normal and language disordered children. journal of communication disorders, 18, 409-427. lozanov, g. & gateva, e. (1989). the foreign language teacher's suggestopedic manual. switzerland: gordon and breach science publishers. luria, a.r. (1982). language and cognition. washington: winston & sons. ornstein, r.e. (1975). the psychology of consciousness. new york: penguin books. penn, c. and cleary, j. (1988). compensatory strategies in the language of closed head injured patients. brain injury, 2, 1, 3-17. powell, g.e. (1981). brain function therapy. great britain: gower. prigatano, g.p. (1986). neuropsychological rehabilitation after brain injury. baltimore: john hopkins university press. rosenzweig, m.r. and bennet, e.l. (1976). neuronal mechanisms of learning and memory. cambridge: mit press. russell, p. (1986). the brain book. london: routledge & kegen paul. scott, b.s. (1976). the effect of elevated potassium on the time course of neuron survival in cultures of dissociated dorsal root ganglia. journal of cellular physiology, 91, 305-316. sperry, r.w. (1982). some effects of disconnecting the cerebral hemispheres. science, 217, 1223-1226. i taylor, o.l. (1986). treatment of communication disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse populations (ed.). california: college-hill press. | van kraayenberg, f. (1992). lecture series: creative accelerative learning methods: lead the field, johannesburg. ylvisaker, m.a. & szekeres, s.f. (1986). management of the patient with closed head injury. in brookshire, r. (ed.), clinical aphasiology conference proceedings. minneapolis: brk publishers. 1 zaidel, e. (1985). roger sperry: an appreciation. in benson, d.f. & zaidel, e. (eds.), the dual brain. new york: the guilford press. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) d i e o n t w i k k e l i n g e n t o e t s i n g v a n a r t i k u l a s i e v e r m o e n s b y d i e a f r i k a a n s s p r e k e n d e k i n d elsie c. lotter,m.a. (log.) (pretoria) spraakheelkunde, universiteit van pretoria, pretoria opsomming daar bestaan tans g e e n gestandaardiseerde afrikaanse a r t i k u l a s i e t o e t s nie en o n t w i k k e l i n g s n o r m s vir die klanke van afrikaans is n o g nie t e v o r e o p g e s t e l nie. in hierdie studie is g e p o o g o m 'n stel o u d e r d o m s n o r m s vir die artikulasiev e r m o e van die afrikaanssprekende kind daar te stel en o m 'n v o o r l o p i g e vorm van 'n afrikaanse a r t i k u l a s i e t o e t s vir gebruik b y j o n g kinders o p te stel. d i e g e g e w e n s van 9 9 9 p r o e f p e r s o n e ( 4 9 9 seuns en 5 0 0 dogters) tussen die ouderd o m m e 3-9 en 9 6 jaar is verwerk vir die vasstelling van d i e o n t w i k k e l i n g s norms. d i e toetsmateriaal vir d i e a r t i k u l a s i e t o e t s is gekies na aanleiding van die r e s p o n s e van 2 3 1 5 p r o e f p e r s o n e . b e n e w e n s 'n w o o r d e l y s vir gebruik as artikulasie-inventaris, w o r d verskeie verwerkings en t o e p a s s i n g s van verkree r e s p o n s e voorgestel. daar w o r d aanbeveel dat die v o o r l o p i g e t o e t s f o r m a a t die v o l g e n d e insluit: 'n tabel vir die o p t e k e n i n g van r e s p o n s e ; g e g e w e n s vir die b e r e k e n i n g van a l g e m e n e spraakverstaanbaarheid; materiaal vir die o n t l o k k i n g van 'n m o n s t e r van a a n e e n l o p e n d e spraak; 'n m e t o d e vir die o n t l e d i n g van f o u t r e s p o n s e ; en 'n s t i m u l e e r b a a r h e i d s s u b t o e t s vir d i a g n o s t i e s e d o e l e i n d e s . summary a t present, there e x i s t s no standardized afrikaans a r t i c u l a t i o n test and dev e l o p m e n t a l n o r m s for t h e s o u n d s of afrikaans have not y e t b e e n established. this s t u d y a t t e m p t s t o provide a set of age n o r m s for t h e articulatory abilities of afrikaans-speaking children and t o formulate a preliminary afrikaans a r t i c u l a t i o n test t o b e used w i t h y o u n g children. data c o l l e c t e d from 9 9 9 subjects, ( 4 9 9 b o y s and 5 0 0 girls) b e t w e e n t h e ages 3-9 a n d 9 6 years, w e r e processed for the e s t a b l i s h m e n t of t h e d e v e l o p m e n t a l norms. t h e material used for t h e articulation test w a s s e l e c t e d a c c o r d i n g t o r e s p o n s e s o b t a i n e d from 2 3 1 5 subjects. in a d d i t i o n to a list of w o r d s t o b e used as an a r t i c u l a t i o n i n v e n t o r y , several c o m p u t a t i o n s and a p p l i c a t i o n s of r e s p o n s e s o b t a i n e d from subjects are p r o p o s e d . it is s u g g e s t e d that t h e preliminary test format i n c l u d e t h e f o l l o w i n g : an i n d e x for t h e r e c o r d i n g o f r e s p o n s e s ; data for t h e c o m p u t a t i o n of general s p e e c h intelligibility; material for eliciting a sample o f cont i n u o u s s p e e c h ; a m e t h o d for t h e analysis of error r e s p o n s e s ; a n d a stimulability s u b t e s t for d i a g n o s t i c purposes. die s t u d i e e n b e h a n d e l i n g v a n a r t i k u l a s i e p r o b l e m e w o r d d i k w e l s a s ' n b e t r e k lik e e n v o u d i g e o n d e r a f d e l i n g van d i e s p r a a k p a t o l o g i e b e s k o u . t e n s p y t e hierv a n v i n d b a i e t e r a p e u t e t o g d a t d i e m e e s t e v a n h u l l e t y d d e u r gevalle m e t a r t i k u l a s i e p r o b l e m e in beslag g e n e e m w o r d , d i k w e l s t e n k o s t e van k i n d e r s w a t b l y k b a a r e r n s t i g e r p r o b l e m e h e t e n h u l p m e e r n o d i g h e t . s t e w a r t 9 w y s e g t e r d a a r o p d a t die t y d w a t n o d i g is o m b e v r e d i g e n d e s p r a a k b y s o m m i g e v a n h i e r d i e k i n d e r s t e v e r k r y , s k y n b a a r b u i t e alle v e r h o u d i n g is m e t d i e o p g a w e s o o s h y h o r n b y d i e e e r s t e o o g o p s l a g v o o r d o e n . tdskrif van i suid-afrikanse vereniging vir sprak n goorhlknde, vol. 21, dsmber 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) •34 elsie c. lotter navorsers besef in 'n toenemende mate dat powers 7 se uitgangspunt aandag verdien: artikulasieprobleme is geensins so eenvoudig om te verklaar en te behandel as wat baie mense aanvaar nie. die instrumente waarmee die terapeut artikulasievermoens kan ondersoek en 'n sinvolle terapieprogram opstel, neem dus 'n belangrike plek in. 'n artikulasie-inventaris bied 'n stelselmatige wyse waarop die klanke wat in die betrokke taal as foneemrealiserings benodig word, nagegaan kan word, 'n gesistematiseerde oorsig van die pasient se produksie van hierdie klanke vergemaklik die beplanning van die terapieprogram. soms is dit ook nodig, veral by baie jong kinders, om na te gaan watter klankproduksies wel al bemeester moes gewees het en of daar sommige ,klankfoute' voorkom wat op daardie ouderdom nog as normaal beskou word. hierdie oorwegings dra ook by tot die opstel van realistiese terapiedoelstellings. ontwikkelingsnorms (ouderdomsnorms) le dus dikwels ten grondslag van die sinvolle interpretasie van 'n persoon se prestasie tydens -n artikulasietoets. ouderdomsnorms word benut in verskillende soorte artikulasietoetse — siftingstoetse, diagnostiese en voorspellingstoetse. 'n stelselmatige aanduiding van artikulasievermoens is egter nie net in terapie nuttig nie, maar ook vir navorsingsdoeleindes. nie 'slegs op die gebied van artikulasieafwykings as sodanig nie, maar ook by studies waar die klem op ander oorwegings val, word die artikulasietoets dikwels gebruik. voorbeelde van sulke studies word oral in die huidige vakliteratuur aangetref. in baie van hierdie gevalle word prestasie op die artikulasietoets juis in terme van ontwikkelingsnorms geinterpreteer. daar kan dus geen twyfel bestaan dat die artikulasietoets of-inventaris, met sy gegewens in verband met ontwikkelingspeile, wel 'n belangrike instrument vir die klinikus en die navorser is nie. wat die ontwikkeling en afwykings van spraak en taal betref, is daar betreklik min navorsing in afrikaans en oor afrikaans gedoen. waar dit wel aangevoer is, is meestal ongestandaardiseerde vertalings en/of aanpassings van engelse en amerikaanse toetse gebruik. die gebruik van sodanige vreemde toetse lewer egter nie werklik geldige of betroubare resultate nie, veral nie met betrekking tot artikulasie nie. elke taal het 'n verskillende foneemstelsel en selfs 'n verskillende artikulasiebasis. waar baie navorsingsprogramme tans gerig is op veral die ontwikkeling van die taalstelsel as geheel en die verband tussen die onderafdelings van taal (wat ook die foneemstelsel en die realisering daarvan insluit) is daar 'n dringende behoefte aan basiese inligting. hierdie inligting moet, vir afrikaans, nog ingesamel word. ' κ in hierdie studie word dan gepoog om: (i) sekere basiese gegewens in verband met artikulasieontwikkeling te voorsien, as grondslag vir verdere navorsing; dit wil se, om ontwikkelingsnorms vir artikulasievermoe by die jong afrikaanssprekende kind vas te stel. (ii) 'n instrument daar te stel waarmee sodanige basiese gegewens by ander individuele en groepe proefpersone ingesamel'kan word, hetsy vir terapeutiese of navorsingsdoeleindes; dit wil se, om 'n voorlopige vorm van 'n afrikaanse artikulasietoets vir gebruik by jong kinders op te stel. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 21, decenivsr 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die toetsing van artikulasievermoens by afrikaanssprekende kind 35 l die navorsing wat onderneem is, het dus ook twee sake behels. in die eerste plek, die toetsing van die klanke wat as foneemrealiserings in afrikaans voorkom, soos dit deur 'n groot groep kinders van verskillende ouderdomme geproduseer word. in die tweede plek, die versameling van 'n lys woorde wat geskik sal wees vir gebruik in 'n artikulasietoets by kinders van verskillende ouderdomme. hierdie twee aspekte is tydens die ondersoek gelyktydig gedek. metode p r o e f p e r s o n e proefpersone vir hierdie studie moes „jong afrikaanssprekende kinders" wees. kinders vanaf die ouderdom 3-9 tot 9-6 is in die ondersoek ingesluit. (navorsingsgegewens uit amerikaans, engels en duits dui daarop dat kinders teen ongeveer 8 jaar reeds al die klanke van hulle taal behoort te kan se. die hipotese kan dus gestel word dat dit ook vir afrikaans die geval sal wees). alle proefpersone is uit afrikaanse skole of kleuterskole getrek. die vereiste van afrikaans as huistaal is ook gestel. kinders met sigbare fisiese afwykings, gehoorverlies of neurologiese afwykings (waar bekend) is nie in die ondersoek opgeneem nie. die ontwikkelingsnorms sal dus geld vir kinders met normale fisiese strukture. 'n totaal van 999 proefpersone (500 dogters, 499 seuns) is gebruik om die ontwikkelingsnorms te bepaal. die gegewens van 2315 kinders is gebruik om die bekendheid van die woorde of prente wat as toetsmateriaal gebruik is, te bepaal. t o e t s m a t e r i a a l die toetsmateriaal moes 'n middel voorsien waardeur die ondersoeker 'n verteenwoordigende monster van elke proefpersoon se spraak kon ontlok. hierdie spraakmonster moes aan sekere vereistes voldoen: dit moes spontaan wees, dit moes verteenwoordigend wees en die verkryging daarvan moes nie te veel tyd in beslag neem nie. uit hierdie vereistes het geblyk dat die toetsmateriaal woorde moes behels wat deur middel van prentstimulasie by jong kinders ontlok kon word en waarin die verlangde klanke dan in die verlangde posisies in woorde (prevokalies, intervokalies, postvokalies) sou voorkom. slegs konsonantklanke (enkelklanke en klankkombinasies) is in hierdie studie gedek; • vokaalklanke is nie ondersoek nie. aangesien dit belangrik was dat kinders goed moes reageer op die prentstimulasie, moes die mees doeltreffende soort prente gebruik word. shanks, sharpe & j a c k s o n 8 vergelyk verskillende soorte prente en bevind dat kinders die beste op gekleurde foto's reageer. die onkoste hieraan verbonde sou egter onprakties wees vir 'n ondersoek van hierdie aard. groot gekleurde tekeninge is dus gebruik waarop voorwerpe so realisties as moontlik uitgebeeld is. by die aanvang van toetsing is 'n voorlopige woordelys en prente saamgestel. tydens die verloop van die ondersoek is hierdie materiaal na behoefte gewysig. as dit geblyk het dat 'n betrokke woord of prent by meer as 50% van die proefpersone wat op een dag ondersoek is, onbekend was, is dit vervang. skrif vi suid-afrikanse vereniging vir sprak n oorlinde, vol. 21, dcsmbr 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 elsie c. lotter die finale woordelys is aangedui in tabel 1. woord % woord % woord % hare 0,0 21. brood .1,2 41. piesang 6,3 broek 0,0 22. blare 1,3 42. vliegtuig 7,7 knoop 0,1 23. wiel 1,6 43. boks 7,9 drink 0,1 24. seuntjie 1,9 44. wolke 8,7 sleutels 0,1 25. stoof 2,0 45. spieel 8,8 skoene 0,1 26. mielie 2,0 46. glas 9,3 mes 0,1 27. stryk 2,7 . 47. pleister 9,7 neus 0,2 28. kar 2,8 48. kwas 10,0 lepel 0,2 29. tafel 2,8 49. vloer 11,6 huis 0,2 30. water 2,9 50. skryf 12,3 vurk 0,2 31. emmer 3,5 51. spring 15,4 druiwe 0,3 32. masjien 3,5 52. jas 15,9 boom 0,3 33. deur 3,5 53. kraan 18,8 perd 0,4 34. geld 3,7 54. lamp 20,4 olifant 0,7 35. voel 3,8 55. prop 23,6 swem 0,8 36. fiets 4,1 56. zebra 31,8 vingers 0,8 37. gras 4,6 57. skulp 32,8 rok 1,0 38. klippe 4,7 58. gholf 42,1 seep 1,0 39. berg 5,2 59. gogga 45,9 trein 1,1 40. baba 5,8 60. beker 60,9 61. sjokolade 62,1 tabel 1. finale woordelys by ondersoek na artikulasievaardighede, tesame met persentasie foutiewe response verkry. a l g e m e n e p r o s e d u r e die toetsmateriaal (woorde) is aangewend om 'n spraakvoorbeeld van elke proefpersoon te verkry. hierdie voorbeeld is dan deur die ondersoeker perseptueel ontleed om die resultate vir die ontwikkelingsnorms te verkry. die groot, gekleurde tekeninge is aangebied vir elke proefpersoon, met toepaslike vrae waar nodig ten einde die verlangde respons te ontlok. (die verlangde woord is nooit in die vraag gebruik nie). die respons van elke proefpersoon op elke prent is aangeteken, vir sowel bepaling van die bekendheid van prente, as beoordeling van artikulasievermoe. journal of the south african spccch and hearing association, vol. 21, december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die toetsing van artikulasievermoens by afrikaanssprekende kind 37 'n getroue fonetiese transkripsie is vir die optekening gebruik. die getranskribeerde response is dan geklassifiseer as aanvaarbaar/nie aanvaarbaar nie, volgens die volwasse standaardpatroon. resultate: ouderdomsnorms die gegewens van 999 proefpersone is verwerk en in tabelvorm uiteengesit. 'n getroue weergawe is ontwikkel deur ibm 360 model 50 komper, met die program „interest" en ook verdere programme wat deur die personeel van die rekenaarsentrum aan die universiteit van pretoria opgestel is. die tabelle word nie hier weergegee nie omdat hulle te lywig is. die volgende is 'n samevatting van die vernaamste bevindings: (a) hoewel die persentasie proefpersone wat die konsonantklanke (enkelklanke en klankkombinasies) korrek produseer, in sommige gevalle met toename in ouderdom 'n stygende neiging toon,'was daar vir geen klank 'n deurlopende styging nie. afnames in die persentasie proefpersone wat 'n korrekte produksie lewer, kom voor in die omgewing van 5-0 tot 5-8, 6-8 tot 7-6 en weer voor 8-8. indien hierdie ondersoek met ander proefpersone en ander toetsmateriaal herhaal sou word en dieselfde resultate oplewer, sal 'n verklaring moontlik gesoek moet word in die verband tussen die ontwikkeling van artikulasievermoe en ander vermoens. 'n voorlopige hipotese kan gestel word in terme van tydperke van intensiewe konseptuele ontwikkeling, waartydens dan minder aandag aan die korrekte realisering van foneme (wat tog op 'n vroeere ouderdom reeds bemeester is) geskenk word. (b) enkelklanke word oor die algemeen vroeer as klankkombinasies bemeester, met' die uitsondering van die enkelklanke [r] en [sj. op die ouderdom van 8 jaar produseer die meeste kinders (75% -90%) al die konsonantklanke van afrikaans korrek, met uitsondering van die klankkombinasie [-rs]. 'n steekproef met 50 volwasse proefpersone het getoon dat 26% van die volwassenes ook 'n foutiewe produksie van die klankkombinasie lewer. die vorms wat gewoonlik voorkom, naamlik [j sj of [ r / ] , word egter algemeen as aanvaarbare variante beskou. (c) soos weerspieel deur die persentasie fouteerders, blyk dit dat [s, r, / , z, g], in daardie volgorde, die vyf moeilikste enkelkonsonantklanke vir kinders binne die ouderdomsgroep 3-9 tot 9-6 was. as hierdie gegewens met die vir amerikaans, engels en duits vergelyk word, is daar 'n groot mate van ooreenstemming, veral ten opsigte van [s, z, / ] . dit blyk dat die sibilante klanke in al vier hierdie tale as moeilik produseerbare klanke ondervind word. (d) wat die moeilikheidswaarde van konsonantklankkombinasies betref, is [-rs] die moeilikste. al tien die moeilikste konsonantklankkombinasies bevat [r], [s] of albei as komponent. (e) dit lyk asof die enkelkonsonantklanke moeiliker produseerbaar is in die eindas in die aanvangsposisie in woorde. 'n verklaring hiervoor kan gevjnd word in die hipotese van mcdonald,5 dat die eindkonsonant van tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 elsie c. lotter 'n lettergreep, as gevolg van die tydsfaktor, in aaneenlopende spraak na die begin van die volgende lettergreep verskuif. die eindkonsonante is gevolglik moontlik minder konstant en moeiliker om aan te leer. (f) wanneer konsonante ten opsigte van manier van vorming met mekaar vergelyk word, kom die grootste persentasie fouteerders op wrywingsklanke voor. die konsonantsoorte kan in volgorde van die grootste na die kleinste persentasie fouteerders so gerangskik word: wrywingsklanke, die ratelof tikklank, die wrywinglose kontinuant met laterale lugvrylating, afsluitingsklanke, wrywinglose, kontinuante met nasale lugvrylating. hierdie bevindings stem ooreen met die van t e m p l i n 1 0 vir amerikaans. die ontwikkelingsnorms wat in hierdie ondersoek vasgestel is, is nuttig vir gebruik in verdere navorsing en ook by die opstel van 'n voorlopige vorm van 'n afrikaanse artikulasietoets. besprek1ng: voorlopige vorm van 'n artikulasietoets by 'n beoordeling van die toetsformaat wat hier voorgestel word, moet die voorwaardes wat deur goldman & f r i s t o e 3 gestel is, veral in gedagte gehou word: 'n artikulasie-inventaris of -toets is slegs doeltreffend as dit akkurate en voldoende inligting oplewer, as dit binne 'n kort tyd toegepas kan word en as dit vir die kind ίι hoe interessantheidswaarde het. die toets moet dus materiaal bevat wat aan die kinders bekend is en.wat hulle aandag sal behou, maar terselfdertyd moet die ondersoeker die materiaal en die verwerking van die resultate maklik kan hanteer. t o e t s m a t e r i a a l die toetsmateriaal sal bestaan uit prente wat so gekies is, dat die kind in die benoeming van die hele stel prente, die enkelkonsonantklanke en konsonantklankkombinasies van afrikaans sal produseer. enkelklanke word in drie posisies in woorde (te wete aanvang, middel en end) ontlok, behalwe in gevalle waar die taalreels spesifiseer dat 'n klank nie in 'n bepaalde woordposisie voorkom nie. die klankkombinasies word elk net in een woordposisie ontlok, na gelang van die aard van die bepaalde klankkombinasie. om die lengte van die toets te beperk, word in sommige gevalle meer as een klank per woord beoordeel. die woordelys wat in tabel 1 aangegee is, kan gebruik word as 'n bron vir die woorde wat in die artikulasie-inventaris opgeneem word. die 54 woorde wat in tabel ii verskyn word aanbeveel. daar word aanbeveel dat verdere ondersoek ingestel word om meer doeltreffende stimuluswoorde vir die volgende gevalle te vind: [k], [x] en [d] in middelposisie van woorde. daar moet ook verder gepoog word om woorde te vind wat die klanke wat nie by hierdie toets ingesluit is nie, te ontlok. sekere klankkombinasies, bv. [spl-, -rf, -rps], rrloes weggelaat word. hierdie klankkombinasies kon moeilik deur middel van prente ontlok word omdat dit nie voorkom in woorde wat aan kinders bekend is, of wat met 'n prent voorgestel kan word en 'n bevredigende respons lewer nie. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 21, decer 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die toetsing van artikulasievermoens by afrikaanssprekende kind 39 1. |>erd 19. olifant 37. glas 2. legel 20. sgieel 38. prop 3. seep 21. rok 39. drink 4. boom 22. hare • 40. kraan 5. baba 23. piesang 41. j>ras 6. mes 24. kat . 42. swem 7. emmer 25. beker 43. kwas 8. vurk 26. geld 44. knoop 9. tafel 27. vliegtuig 45. spring 10. stoof 28. gholf 46. stryk 11. water 29. masjien 47. skryf 12. druiwe 30. vingers 48. skulp 13. brood 3 1 . j a s 49. wolke 14. deur 32. pleister 50. berg 15. sjokolade· 33. zebra 51. fiets 16. neus 34. blare 52. boks 17.skoene 35. sleutels 53. seuntjie 18. trein 36. klippe 54. lamp tabel ii. aanbevole woordlys vir artikulasieinventaris. die onderstreepte gedeeltes van die woorde in die aanbevole lys, dui op die klanke wat tydens produksie van die bepaalde woord geevalueer word. daar word egter aanbeveel dat die ondersoeker ook op elke ander realisering van die betrokke klank wat in die res van die toets mag voorkom, sal let. optekening v a n response daar word aanbeveel dat elke uiting van die toetsling so getrou as moontlik foneties getranskribeer word, met bykomstige aantekeninge en/of diakritiese tekens waar nodig. sodoende word die grootste moontlike hoeveelheid inligting vir verdere interpretasie voorsien. 'n tabel soos die wat deur goldman & fristoe 3 voorsien word, kan dien om . 'n stelselmatige oorsig van die toetsling se response op die artikulasieinventaris te gee. in hierdie tabel is die klanke volgens ontwikkelingsvolgorde (soos uit hierdie navorsing verkry is) gerangskik. die ouderdomsnorms kan by d i e , toetsformaat ingesluit word, sodat die response van die toetsling daarmee vergelyk kan word. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheekunde, vol. 21, dsember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 elsie c. lotter bykomstige verwerking v a n gegewens na 'n noukeurige bestudering van die massa literatuur wat reeds oor die onderwerp artikulasietoetsing verskyn het, is 'n paar aanbevelings vir die bykomstige verwerking van gegewens uitgelig. die volgende het geblyk die mees belowende en nuttige moontlikhede te wees: (a) artikulasievaardigheid in aaneenlopende spraak: die belang van 'n indruk van die toetsling se artikulasievaardigheid in aaneenlopende spraak, word allerwee beklemtoon. twee maatstawwe kan hier gebruik word: (i) 'n numeriese punt as aanduiding van algemene spraakverstaanbaarheid, soos aanbeveel deur barker1 en barker & england.2 hierdie punt word gegrond op die voorkomsfrekwensie van klanke; as die kind 'n aantal klanke met hoe voorkomsfrekwensie foutief produseer, sal dit sy spraak meer onverstaanbaar maak as wanneer hy 'n aantal klanke met lae voorkomsfrekwensie foutief sou produseer. die gegewens van odendal 6 kan gebruik word om so 'n puntetoekenning vir die klanke van afrikaans te bereken. (ii) om 'n perseptuele indruk van die toetsling se aaneenlopende spraak te verkry, kan 'n verhalende toets soos die van goldman & fristoe 3 opgestel word. die enkelklanke en klankkombinasies wat vir die totale groep kinders in die huidige ondersoek die moeilikste was, kan in so 'n verhalende toets ingesluit word. 'n stel sinne, waarby toepaslike prente gebruik kan word, is ook reeds opgestel; die praktiese toepasbaarheid daarvan moet egter nog deur verdere navorsing vasgestel word. eienskappe % θ s medio-alveoler / χ / bladlinguaal / χ / medio-orale lugvrylating x / /. stemloos / / / frikatief / / / eienskappe teenwoordig 4 : 3 / 5 tabel iii. fonetiese eienskappe van [s], [θ] en [ £ ]. (b) aard van foute: die artikulatoriese kenmerke van elke klank, enkelklanke sowel as klankkombinasies, kan by die toetsvorm ingesluit word. die artikulatoriese kenmerke van die toetsling se foutklanke kan dan met die journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 21, decer 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die toetsing van artikulasievermoens by afrikaanssprekende kind 41 standaardpatroon vergelyk word, soos voorgestel deur hutcheson. 4 uit so, 'n ontleding kan afleidings gemaak word in verband met besondere gebiede van uitval, byvoorbeeld verskille tussen die realisering van 'n foneem in verskillende woordposisies, patroon van gebruik van sekere artikulatoriese eienskappe, en so meer. tabel iii is 'n voorbeeld van die vergelyking van die fonetiese (artikulatoriese) eienskappe van [s] met die foutiewe realiserings [θ] en [ $ ]. (c)' stimuleerbaarheid as diagnostiese maatstaf: w i n i t z 1 1 berig dat artikulasieverbetering met die grootste mate van betroubaarheid voorspel kan word op grond van reaksie op stimulasie met die korrekte klankpatroon en die toetsling se telling op die aanvanklike artikulasietoets. 'n stimuleerbaarheidssubtoets, soortgelyk aan die wat deur goldman & fristoe 3 aangegee word, kan dus by die afrikaanse artikulasietoets ingesluit word. wanneer die afrikaanse artikulasietoets in sy finale vorm opgestel word, , sal die resultate uit die navorsing soos hierbo uiteengesit, hopelik nuttig . gebruik kan word. as hierdie studie kan dien om verdere navorsing ten opsigte van die bree taalspektrum van afrikaans te stimuleer, het dit in sy doel geslaag. erkenning hierdie navorsing is moontlik gemaak deur die geldelike steun van die raad vir geesteswetenskaplike navorsing. enige menings wat uitgespreek word, is egter uitsluitlik die van die skryfster. verwysings 1. barker, j. (1960): a numerical measure of articulation./. speech & hearingdis., 25, 79-88. 2. barker, j., & england, g. (1962): a numerical measure of articulation: further developments. / . speech & hearing dis., 27, 23-27. 3. goldman, r. & fristoe, μ. (1969): goldman-fristoe test of articulation. american guidance service, inc., minnesota. 4. hutcheson, s. (1968): some quantitative and qualitative criteria in articulation test scoring. brit. j. dis. commun., 3, 36-42. 5. mcdonald, e.t. (\969): articulation testing and treatment. a sensorymo tor approach. stanwix house inc., pittsburgh. • 6. odendal, f.f. (1962) : die struktuur van die afrikaanse wortelmorfeem. h.a.u.m., kaapstad en pretoria. 7. powers, m.h. (1957): functional disorders of articulation. chapt. 23, in handbook of speech pathology, appleton-century-crofts, new york. 8. shanks, s.j., sharpe, m.r. & jackson, b.r. (1970): spontaneous responses of first grade children to diagnostic picture articulation tests. / . commun. dis., 3, 106-117. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheekunde vol. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 elsie c. lotter 9. stewart, a. (1968): disorders of articulation in children. brit. j. dis. commun., 3,121-129. 10. templin, μ .c. (19 5 7): certain language skills in children. university of minnesota press, minneapolis. 11. travis, l.e. (ed.). (1957): handbook of speech pathology. appletoncentury-crofts, new york. 12. winitz, h.'(1969) : articulatory acquisition and behavior. appletoncentury-crofts, new york. for all y o u r m e d i c a l book a n d j o u r n a l r e q u i r e m e n t s consult the specialists p. b. mayer 902 n o r w i c h house heerengracht cape t o w n telephone 2-9231 journal oj' the south aj'riean speech and hearing association, vol. 21, december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) basf introduces the jampreof c120 cassette. basf the first people in the world to make recording tape have now patented a brilliantly simple, foolproof improvement for the innards of cassettes. it's called special mechanics and, briefly, this is how it works :in all new cassettes the neatly reeled tape fits comfortably between the top and bottom of the cassette case. the hubs turn freely. but, after it's unreeled and reeled up again a few times the tape may not wind back as neatly. so that the reel of tape gets fatter and doesn't f i t comfortabiy any more. it starts to drag. and the winding up side goes slower than the unwinding side. a n d . . . need we go on? special mechanics consists of two plastic "tusks" that guide the tape on and off the hubs. so that it stays neat, the spools keep turning freely, and the music glides smoothly on at a serene 11 i.p.s. sm also incorporated in basf chromdioxide c60, c90 and c120 cassettes. special mechanics is guaranteed to give you smoother, trou blefree recording and playback. or we'll give you a free replacement cassette. basf special mechanics sm spezial mechanik ui&kiotoxlkx*rlck&llowi·' 2983 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheeikunde, vol. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) philips hearing aid services a division of s.a. philips (pty) ltd. hearing aids portable audiometers group teaching systems p h i l i p s hearing aid services head office 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street, p.o. box 3069, johannesburg. p h i l i p s @ ) 4 4 9 2 3 journal o the south african spe and hearing association, vol. 21, december 974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 31 interaction between a teacher and the non-speaking as well as speaking children in the classroom elsa popich and erna alant centre for augmentative and alternative communication, department of communication pathology, university of pretoria abstract this study examined the verbal interactions which occurred between a teacher and two groups of children (children who were non-speaking as well as children who were speaking). descriptive data, generated by analysing ten lessons, suggested that the teacher's interaction with the children who were non-speaking differed, in terms of quantity and quality. she directed approximately 10% less interaction at each of the three non-speaking children, when compared with the number of interactions that she directed at each of the five speaking children. however, she did not spend an equal amount of time interacting with each of the non-speaking children. her interaction with the non-speaking children was dominated by questions, attention directing and requesting. verbalization types, such as answering and imitating did not occur at all in the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children. this implies that the non-speaking children's learning experiences in the classroom differed from the speaking children's learning experiences. possible reasons for these discrepancies were proposed, namely that the teacher's attitudes, skill and knowledge played a role, but the non-speaking children's lack of access to communication was also considered to be a factor in determining the amount and type of interaction. opsomming hierdie studie het die verbale interaksie tussen 'n onderwyseres en twee groepe kinders(kinders wat nie-sprekend is sowel as kinders wat sprekend is) bestudeer. beskrywende data wat gegenereer is deur die analise van tien lesse, suggereer dat die onderwyseres se interaksie met die nie-sprekende kinders verskil het in terme van hoeveelheid en kwaliteit. sy het ongeveer 10% minder interaksie aan elkeen van die drie nie-sprekende kinders gerig in vergelyking met die aantal interaksies wat sy aan elkeen van die vyf sprekende kinders gerig het. sy het egter nie ewe veel gepraat met elkeen van die nie-sprekende kinders nie. haar interaksie met die nie-sprekende kinders het oorwegend uit vrae, aandag rig en versoeke bestaan. verbalisasies, soos antwoorde en nabootsing het nie voorgekom in die onderwyseres se interaksie met die nie-sprekende kinders nie. dit impliseer dat die nie-sprekende kinders se leerervarings in die klas verskil van die sprekende kinders se leerervarings. moontlike redes vir hierdie verskille was voorgestel, naamlik dat die onderwyseres se houdings, vermoe en kennis 'n rol speel, maar dat die nie-sprekende kinders se beperkte toegang tot kommunikasie ook 'n deurslaggewende rol in die bepaling van die hoeveelheid en tipe interaksie, gespeel het. keywords: classroom interaction; non-speaking; verbalization types. ) when a child has difficulty in actively participating in class, the problems the child experiences could be due to either the fact that the child does not have access to communication or that the child is not given the opportunity to interact (beukelman & mirenda, 1992). although one can separate factors relating to access and opportunities for communication, these aspects are interrelated. the mere provision of a means to gain access to interaction is not sufficient to increase the participation of the nonspeaking child. training of potential communication partners in the provision of interaction opportunities forms an important additional component of the intervention process (calculator & luchko, 1983). limited access to communication is related to the mode of communication the child uses (beukelman & mirenda, 1992). for the non-speaking child, who cannot meet all of his communication needs through speech, another mode should be considered in order to increase access to communication, for example the use of an alternative and augmentative communication (aac) system. providing a suitable system to facilitate interaction means that the system should aim not only to increase the intelligibility of the communication attempts but also the rate of message transmission and the child's general access to a portable communication system (beukelman & yorkston, 1982). providing the correct aac system is thus important to ensure participation in the classroom in meaningful interactions. however, only providing the child with access to communication does not ensure an increase in learning (beukelman, 1991). due to the fact that aac users in the classroom primarily occupy a respondent role it is equally important for learning that the child not only die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 elsa popich & erna alant be given access to communication but also be provided with the opportunity to interact (dalton & bedrosian, 1989). opportunity barriers are, therefore, an important aspect to be considered in the description of classroom interaction and teachers will have to assume at least some of the responsibility when aac users fail to interact (mirenda & donnellan, 1986). the extent to which a child is included in classroom interaction is affected by the teacher creating opportunities for the child to communicate (beukelman & mirenda, 1992). the less the child's speaking abilities, the more difficult for the teacher to ensure the child's participation in interaction. factors such as the teacher's attitude towards the non-speaking child, the teacher's knowledge of the importance of interaction for the increasing intelligibility of the non-speaking child and the teacher's skill in including the non-speaking child in interaction; will determine the success with which the non-speaking child is included in classroom interaction. teaching strategies that do not actively demand the participation of the aac user will result in increasing passivity in the non-speaking child (bottorf & depape, 1982). research has indicated that aac users in the classroom situation seldom initiate interaction (basil, 1992) and that this lack of social interaction often results in a decrease in language competence and in fewer learning opportunities being available to the non-speaking child (grayshon, 1977). in spite of evidence that a high level of responsiveness on the part of the teacher will have a positive influence on the child's conversational abilities (mirenda & donnellan, 1986), it was found that more than fifty percent of the initiations made by mentally handicapped pupils to teachers were not responded to (beveridge & hurrel, 1980). factors contributing to the frequency and quality of teachers responses to the non-speaking children are three-fold. firstly, the teacher's lack of responsiveness can be attributed to the teacher's expectations of the individual child (low or high achiever) with more opportunities for interaction being offered to the higher-achieving individual (light & mcnaughton, 1993). secondly, the teacher may limit the number of interactions with the child due to the fact that she cannot predict the child's capabilities in performing certain tasks in interaction (light & mcnaughton, 1993). therefore, the non-speaking child may undertable 1: the levels of verbal expression of the subj achieve, which could lead to the teacher giving him even fewer opportunities due to her lowered expectations of him. thirdly, the teacher's responses to child-initiated interaction are related to the personality of the child, for example, the presence of disruptive behaviour could lead to the child being given fewer opportunities to interact (beveridge, ramsden & leudar, 1989). although aggression is often seen in non-speaking children due to the frustration of not being understood the abscence of aggression is crucial so as not to result in exclusion from communication (baumgart, johnson & helmsetter, 1990). it would seem that a vicious circle could develop, with teachers giving fewer opportunities to non-speaking children, and the children interacting less as a consequence of the teacher's lack of responsiveness to them. not only does the teacher influence the child's participation, but there is evidence that the quantity and quality of an adult's verbal behaviour is related to the verbal output of the child with whom they are interacting (mirenda & donnellan, 1986). interaction can be seen as a pattern of mutual influence and adjustment (malamahthomas, 1988). consequently, the more verbal the child is, the more frequent the interaction with the teacher. in an evaluation of the frequency of the interaction between a teacher and the speaking, as well as non-speaking members of the class, one could therefore expect the teacher to interact more with the children who have a higher verbal output. as mentioned previously, the amount of opportunities the teacher gives to the non-speaking children, will have an important influence on their level of participation and learning. various studies have indicated that not only the quantity, but the quality of interactions between the non-speaking child and the teacher is different from the interactions between the speaking child and the teacher (cicognani & zani, 1992). the degree of impairment of verbal abilities in a child affects the adult language used in communication. teachers' interactions with children with disabilities mainly consisted of instructions, statements and questions while interaction types such as affirming and praise seldom occurred (harris, 1982; klein & harris, 1986). furthermore the questions that were directed at children with disabilities were mostly questions that only required a yes/no response (lossing, yorkston & beukelman, 1985). these findings indicate a possible trend in terms of differences in the frequency and quality of ingroup raw score on the expressive one word picture vocabulary test (gardner, 1979) average percentage: ' communication assessment 1 schedule for severely handicapped students (wium and alant, 1993) group one (non-speaking) 0 1 4 0 5 % group two ' (highly verbal) above 20 75 100% / group three (miscellaneous) 15 1 9 10 70% due to the fact that the expressive one word picture vocabulary test (gardner, 1979) was standardised on normal children, the subjects (all mentally handicapped) could only be compared by using the raw scores as stanines placed them all in the same category. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) interaction between teacher and the non-speaking as well as speaking children in the classroom 33 teractions between teachers and the speaking as well as non-speaking children in their classes. this study focuses on the teacher's interaction with nonspeaking children in the class. the definition of non-speaking has elicited much controversy (matas, mathy-laikko, beukelman & legresley, 1985). for the purpose of this study the term refers to children with less than 15 intelligible words so as to exclude those who are beginning with functional v e r b a l c o m m u n i c a t i o n (burd, hannes, bornhoeft & fisher, 1988). the purpose of this study was to describe the opportunities for interaction, offered to the non-speaking children in a classroom setting, in order to determine whether the teacher's interaction with the nonspeaking children differed in terms of quantity or quality from the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children. method: aim: the goal of this study was to describe the interaction between a primary school teacher and the non-speaking as well as speaking children in the class, during the presentation of various lessons. the subgoals were firstly to determine the number of interactions the teacher directed at ,the non-speaking children in comparison with the number of interactions directed at the speaking children and secondly to determine the types of verbalizations that the teacher directed at the non-speaking children. subjects: the study was conducted at a school for children with impaired cognitive abilities. the school is primarily afrikaans-speaking and has three levels, namely a beginner's phase, a junior phase and a senior phase. a class in the junior phase was selected in consultation with the principal. all the children in the class had normal vision, normal hearing and sufficient'motor abilities in order to function independently in thejclassroom. at least one parent of each subject was receiving a fixed income and all the children in group 1 (non-speaking) or group 2 (speaking) spoke afrikaans as a home language. , the speech and functional communication of each child ; was evaluated using the peabody picture vocabulary test ϋ revised (dunn & dunn, 1981), the expressive one word λ picture vocabulary test (gardner, 1979) as well as the « communication assessment schedule for severely handi g capped students (wium & alant, 1993). according to the children's results on the expressive jone word picture vocabulary test (gardner, 1979) as well % as the communication assessment schedule for severely s handicapped students (wium & alant, 1993) the class was (divided into three groups, namely a non-speaking group, ft a highly verbal group and a third group which consisted ο of children who did not fulfil the requirements for either group one or two. table 1 displays the levels of verbal exg pression of the subjects in each group and table 2 gives a g description of the children in the class. 3 from the criteria, specified in table 1, for the inclusion into group one or group two it is clear that group one and two differ significantly in terms of their levels of verbal ^ expression. ^ ^ co £> ο η co [-" 36 ( m ) ih m ar ri ed en gl is h rh m en ta lly ha nd ica pp ed n on e j j η ο co η-1 s co pl, £> ο w ο κ cd oo" 29 ( m ) ih m ar ri ed p or tu gu es e rh m en ta lly ha nd ica pp ed n on e j j η ο co η-1 s co pl, £> ο w ο ο in t>" c ou ld n ot ih d iv or ce d af ri ka an s rh m en ta lly ha nd ica pp ed e pi le ps y η 00 40 ( m ) co m ar ri ed af ri ka an s rh m en ta lly ha nd ica pp ed e pi le ps y g r o u p 2 : s p e a k in g ω <35 cd" 65 ( m i) ih m ar ri ed af ri ka an s ih m en ta lly ha nd ica pp ed n on e g r o u p 2 : s p e a k in g ο < e . [ " 51 ( m i) τ ί m ar ri ed af ri ka an s rh h yp er ac ti vi ty n on e g r o u p 2 : s p e a k in g m 00 [ " 57 ( m i) ih m ar ri ed af ri ka an s rh m en ta lly ha nd ica pp ed n on e g r o u p 2 : s p e a k in g > -d a) λ ο ε s die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 elsa popich & erna alant procedure: to determine the procedure needed to evaluate classroom interaction a pilot study was conducted. the aims of the pilot study were to determine the recording equipment, as well as the recording procedures and analytical procedures to be utilised in the main study. according to the results of the pilot study the following procedures were determined for the main study. pre-recording procedures: the researcher interacted with the children during teatime and playtime to allow them to familiarise themselves with her, in order to minimize the effect of her presence in the classroom. the teacher was informed that the purpose of the study was to determine the need for aac in her classroom with the possible aim of intervention in the school. the teacher was asked to continue with her lessons as usual, while the researcher observed and audiotaped them. two types of lessons were recorded, namely, perception training and scholastic abilities training. perceptual training classes were concerned with the development of skills such as visual perception and auditory processing whereas scholastic abilities classes were concerned with academic skills such as maths and geography. recording procedures: audio-recordings were made using a cassette recorder and a condenser lapel microphone. each recording was twenty minutes long. recordings were made on tuesdays and wednesdays for five consecutive weeks. ten recordings were made for each of the two types of lessons. the researcher set up the recording equipment and attached a lapel microphone to the teacher's collar. when all the children were seated in a half circle in front of the teacher, the researcher switched on the recording equipment. the researcher sat behind the children, looking towards the teacher and observed the teacher presenting the lesson. after twenty minutes (when the lesson was over), the researcher switched off the recording equipment and removed the lapel microphone from the teacher's collar. while the teacher continued with the day's work, the researcher listened to the audio recording to determine if it was clear to whom each statement had been addressed. the researcher consulted the teacher when it was uncertain. at a later stage, the audio recordings were transcribed and analysed verbatim. the materials used during recording were an aiwa condenser lapel microphone as well as a philips d6280 cassette recorder. transcription procedures: the audio soundtrack was transcribed verbatim and checked with the teacher in order to clarify at whom each statement had been addressed. the external rater simultaneously made an independent transcription and analysis, and points of disagreement were reconsidered until 100% agreement was achieved. in order to ensure that the raw data was transcribed consistently correctly, by both transcribers, certain transcription rules were followed (stuart, vanderhoof & beukelman, 1993), namely: repetitions of words were included; vocalizations that were not actual words were represented and transcribed in a consistent form, e.g., mhmmm, uhhuh, huhuh, mmm, uh, ah aw, whoop; numbers were typed as proper nouns; contractions were typed as such that the proper form was spelled out only when it was spoken that way, e.g., don't was typed as don't and do not was typed as do not; standard abbreviations were included, e.g., dr./mrs.; during the transcription of a communication segment that was unintelligible and the entire segment was skipped even when a few intelligible words were available. a spell checker was used on all transcripts before proceeding with further analysis. analysis of data: quantitative data was established by determining at whom each interaction had been directed and calculating frequencies and proportions. the total number of interactions that the teacher directed at each of the non-speaking children, was ascertained in order to compare the amount of interaction directed at the speaking and the non-speaking children. qualitative data was obtained by defining twelve types of teacher verbalizations (adapted from romski, sevcik, reumann & pate, 1989) and categorising the teachers verbalizations accordingly. the twelve types of verbalizations are as follows: 1. questioning: a sentence adapted by order of words, punctuation or intonation to elicit information, (e.g., is it a shirt? sharon? tell me ...) 2. attention directing: to guide the child's thoughts to a specific topic, (e.g., look here.) 3. answering: a reply to a child's verbalization and not merely negating or affirming, (e.g., yes, it is a shirt.) 4. requesting·. asking for an action, object or comment, (e.g., fetch me the cow.) 5. imitating (the child)·. to mimic the child's utterance with or without expansion, (e.g., hard.) 6. naming·. to designate an object or action, (e.g., it's a shirt.) 7. negating: to imply that what the child said or did was wrong, (e.g., no, it's not a shirt., uh-uh.) 8. affirming: to imply that what the child said or did was right, (e.g., you are very clever.) 9. greeting: a salutation, (e.g., hello friends.) 10. self-repetition: to reproduce their own verbalizations in more or less the same form, (e.g., what is it? what is it?) 11. informative: to state facts or opinions about a certain topic, (e.g., they run with these pants.) 12. uncodable: the utterance does not fall into one of the above communication function categories, (e.g., huh? incomplete sentences). the types of verbalizations directed at group 1 as well as the types of verbalizations directed at group 2 were ascertained, during perceptual as well as scholastic classes. in order to increase the reliability of scoring, the researcher's transcription and analysis of the recordings were checked by an external controller. final decisions were based on 100% agreement between the raters. in order to determine the consistency of the results the change over five weeks, in the types of verbalizations that the teacher directed at group 1, as well as group 2, was determined. at-test was completed to determine if there was a sigthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) interaction between teacher and the non-speaking as well as speaking children in the classroom 35 nificant difference between the proportions, and finally the total proportion of time that the teacher spent in interaction with each of the non-speaking children was ascertained. in order to reveal if the results were consistent over time the change over five weeks in the proportion of time that the teacher spent in interaction with group 1 as well as group 2 was determined. results and discussion the results will be discussed with reference to the aims, namely, the number of interactions directed at each child as well as the types of verbalizations used during interaction. the proportions of interaction figure 1 displays the relative amount (%) of interaction that the teacher directed at each of the subject groups. qrp 2: speaking qrp 1: non-speaking 3% qrp 3: miscellaneous 4185 figure 1: a global view of the amount of interaction directed at the three groups within the class. the teacher directed 41% of the interactions at the whole class as well as theichildren in group three, 56% of the interactions at the speaking children and 3% of the interactions at the non-speaking children. the teacher only directed approximately 1% of her interactions towards each non-speaking child, while directing approximately 11% of her interactions towards each speaking child. this means that approximately 10% less interactions were being directed at each of the non-speaking children, in comparison with the number of interactions directed at each of the speaking children. the above-mentioned figures are only hypothetical proportions however, as they are based on the assumption that the teacher interacted equally with each child in a specific group. a t-test was completed to determine if the two groups (non-speaking children and speaking children) differed significantly. the variable being compared, in the two groups, was the number of interactions directed at the two groups of children by the teacher. the t-test result was 3,31, which is larger than 2,447, resulting in the null hypothesis (the null hypothesis supposes that there is no significant difference) being rejected. therefore it is clear that there is a significant difference between the amount of interaction directed at the two groups. the teacher interacted significantly less with the non-speaking children, than with the speaking children. these findings coincide with the findings of previous studies that the number of interactions uttered by the teacher is affected by the child's level of verbal output (mirenda & donnellan, 1986). this is noteworthy, as it has been shown that a lack of interaction with the teacher could lead to the isolation of the child in the class, a lack of participation, and consequently to the development of passivity in the non-speaking child (basil, 1992). as the non-speaking child is being given less opportunities to participate, he is therefore also being limited in terms of developing and practising new skills. literature proposes that learning in the non-speaking child is reduced, due to insufficient opportunities for interaction being offered to the child (musselwhite & st. louis, 1989). an increase in the amount of verbalizations, directed at the non-speaking child will, however, not necessarily guarantee an increase in learning. to ensure participation and learning the child needs to have a means by which to interact in the classroom and, for that reason, an alternative communication system should be made available to the child (musselwhite & st. louis, 1989). the teacher directed less interactions at the non-speaking group in comparison with the amount of interactions directed at the speaking group. in order to determine whether the teacher interacted equally seldom with all the children in the non-speaking group, her interaction with each of the non-speaking children was analysed individually figure 2 displays the relative amount (%) of interactions that the teacher directed at each of the nonspeaking children (group 1). figure 2: a global view of the amount of interaction directed at each of the non-speaking children. out of the 36 interactions directed at the three children in group 1, 83% of the interactions were directed at child f, 17% of the interactions were directed at child i and 0% of the interactions were directed at child j. this means that there is not only a discrepancy between the amount of interactions that the teacher directed at each of the two groups (non-speaking children and speaking children), but that there is also a discrepancy between the number of interactions that the teacher directed at each of the non-speaking children. this opens the door to the possibility that more issues are at stake, than merely the question of verbal abilities, and these issues could influence the teacher's interaction with each of the speaking children as well. possible reasons, for the difference in the amount of interaction directed at each of the non-speaking children are, the child's personality (beveridge, ramsden & leudar, 1989), the teacher's expectations of the child as well as the teacher's ability to predict the child's capabilities in interaction (light & mcnaughton, 1993), and finally, the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 elsa popich & erna alant child's communicative abilities (mirenda & donnellan, 1986). the presence of aggression and disruptive behaviour in a child has been shown to have a negative influence on the teacher's interaction with that specific individual (beveridge et al., 1989). in this study two non-speaking children, at whom the teacher directed the least number of interactions, had behavioural problems (hitting and biting), whereas the third non-speaking child did not. however, one must consider that the teacher only interacted with all three of the non-speaking children for 3% of the total interaction time while she interacted with each of the speaking children for approximately 10% of the total time. this means that, although the child's personality may influence the teachers interaction, the child's verbal ability will have a far greater influence. from figure 1 it is apparent that the teacher spent only a small proportion of time in interaction with the nonspeaking children. a further analysis was done in order to determine the impact of the lesson content on the amount of interaction directed at the non-speaking as well as speaking children. figures 3 and 4 display the relative amount (%) of interaction that the teacher directed at each of the subject groups, during perceptual and scholastic classes respectively. this allows for a comparison of the teachers interaction with the two groups of children, during different class activities. during perceptual classes only 2% of the interactions were directed at the non-speaking group of children, but 5% of the interactions were directed at the non-speaking group during scholastic classes. although there is a relatively small difference, it is still significant, because the teacher interacts with the non-speaking children for such a small proportion of time. this means that the non-speaking group of children got more than twice the stimulation during scholastic classes, in comparison with perceptual classes. in order to determine whether the results were representative of the classroom interaction a further analysis was done to determine the consistency of the results over time. table 3 displays the amount of interaction directed at the non-speaking children, over a period of five weeks. from the global proportions, depicted in table 3, one can see that the values obtained for the non-speaking children do not show much variation over time, and the results obtained were a true reflection of the interaction patterns in the classroom. a trend becomes apparent, namely that the teacher consistently spent a small proportion of the total amount of time in interaction with the non-speaking children. when comparing the proportions obtained in the perceptual classes to the proportions obtained in the scholastic classes, there is consistently more interaction during the scholastic classes, with one exception. during the third week the trend was reversed, with considerably more time spent in interaction during the perceptual class, than during the scholastic class. in order to establish whether the teacher merely spent less time in interaction with the non-speaking children, or whether there were differences in terms of the quality of interaction directed at the two groups of children, the types of verbalizations directed at the non-speaking as well as speaking children were also determined. the types of verbalizations used in interaction in the analysis of the classroom interactions each of the teacher's utterances was categorised into one of twelve figure 3: the amount of interaction directed at the speaking children and the non-speaking children during perceptual classes. figure 4: the amount of interaction directed at the speaking children and the non-speaking children during scholastic classes. table 3: the amount (%) of interaction that the teacher directed at group 1 (three non-speaking children), over a period of five weeks. lessons week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 ^average χ global view 1,93% 4,23% 2,27% 4,58% 1,08% 0.03 perceptual classes 1,92% 1,76% 3,55% 2,13% / 0 0.02 scholastic classes 1,94% 8,89% 0 8,08% 2,29% 0.05 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) interaction between teacher and the non-speaking as well as speaking children in the classroom 37 types of verbalizations. figure 5 displays how many times each of the 12 types of verbalizations occurred in the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking group of children, as well as in the teacher's interaction with the speaking group. as displayed in figure 5, only 7 of the 12 types of verbalizations occurred in the teacher's interactions with the non-speaking group of children. type 1 (questioning) occurred the most (11 times), followed by type 2 (attention directing) which occurred 8 times and type 4 (requesting) which occurred 7 times. type 6 (naming), type 7 (negating), type 8 (affirming) and type 11 (informative) also occurred. type 3 (answering the child) and type 5 (verbally imitating the child) did not occur at all. this means that the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children was dominated by questions, attention directing and requests. one can see that the teacher's interaction with the speaking children did not follow the same trends in terms of which types of verbalizations that occurred the most. type 1 (questioning) still occurred the most (198 times), but type 8 (affirming) and not type 2 (attention directing) occurred second most. type 4 (requesting) still occurred the third most. one can therefore determine a general trend in the class, namely, that questioning and requesting occur frequently, but that attention directing occurs much more frequently in the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children. the fact that type 11 (informative) occurs much more frequently in the teacher's interaction with the speaking children than in the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children is also of interest due to the implications for the non-speaking child's opportunity for learning in the class. the quality of the teacher's interaction was, therefore, also influenced by the children's verbal abilities. the teacher used a limited variety of utterances when in interaction with the non-speaking children. this is of par19 β _za_ 16 β β 10 11 12 i non-tpeaklnfl wa (peaking t y p e · of verbalization· key: types of verbalizations 1. questioning 2. attention directing 3. answering 4. requesting 5. imitating 6. naming 7. negating 8. affirming 9. greeting 10. self-repetition 11. informative 12. uncodable figure 5: a global view of the types of verbalizations directed at the speaking and non-speaking children. ticular relevance in the classroom situation, because the teacher's input strategies will greatly influence the child's subsequent communicative performance (mirenda & donnellan, 1986). the child's verbal disability should, therefore, be considered in relationship to the interaction partner's behaviour, rather than in absolute terms (mirenda & donellan, 1986). the teacher's verbalizations, directed at the non-speaking children, consisted of a large proportion of questions, attention directing and requests. this confirms the literature which states that teachers' interactions with non-speaking children are characterised by the high frequency of questions (klein & harris, 1986). this type of interaction is characteristic of a directive conversational style, and may further inhibit the interaction with the non-speaking child (mirenda & donellan, 1986). consequently, the question arises of why the teacher used so many questions with the non-speaking children. hills (1986) proposes that it is a technique used by teachers to elicit responses from children and to avoid awkward silences. it has also been hypothesised that it could be a compensatory strategy on the teachers' behalf, to compensate for the child's conversational difficulties (mirenda & donellan, 1986). the high frequency of verbalization type 4 (requesting) is not surprising, as requesting for actions or objects does not require a verbal response from the child. the fact that type 3 (answering the child) or type 5 (verbally imitating the child's verbalizations) did not occur is also to be expected, as the teacher cannot answer or imitate a child who does not speak. the frequent occurrence of affirming (type 8 verbalization) in the teacher's interaction with the speaking children could be attributed to the fact that the speaking children were more involved in the lessons, and therefore had more opportunities to give the correct response. figures 6 and 7 display the types of verbalizations that the teacher directed at the non-speaking children, as well as speaking children, during perceptual and scholastic classes respectively. from figure 5 it is apparent that the teachers' interaction with the two groups of children was characterised by certain trends. a further analysis was done to determine the impact of the lesson content on the types of verbalizations directed at the non-speaking children. " the same trends are seen in the two types of classes as were noted in general, for the non-speaking group, except for the following: type 6 (naming), type 8 (affirming) and type 11 (informative) did not occur in the perceptual classes and there was a relatively large number of type 11 (informative) verbalizations during scholastic classes. a greater proportion of the teacher's interactions with the non-speaking children, occurred during scholastic classes. perceptual skills already start to develop in sensorimotor stage one (louw, 1990) and the skills required from the children during the classes were on a very basic level (e.g., differentiate between loud and soft). however, the scholastic classes required a higher level of functioning and the teacher therefore had to instruct the children more frequently. in order to determine whether the results depicted in figures 5, 6, and 7 were consistent and representational of all five weeks, table 4 compares the results obtained for each of the five weeks. any changes over time, would then become apparent. from the results, depicted in table 4, one can see that die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 the trend discussed previously (the high frequency of questioning, attention directing and requesting in the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children) is indicated for all five weeks. however, in week 4 there is also a relatively high frequency of informative verbalizations elsa popich & erna alant directed at the non-speaking children. this means that the trend is constant over time, but there is some variation. from the results discussed, it becomes clear that the teacher's interaction with the two groups differed in terms 180 f 100 r u s « ο y «ο 10 ο 1 8 3 4 8 8 7 8 9 1 0 1 1 1 2 bnen-apaaklno e2 wmklng typaa of verbalization· key: types of verbalizations 1. questioning 7. negating 2. attention directing 8. affirming 3. answering 9. greeting 4. requesting 10. self-repetition 5. imitating 11. informative 6. naming 12. uncodable 71 1 i 9 aa j 4 1 1 i 1 9 9 ? ι 1 1 1 8 1 j i l l · i 0> i a a 4 b a r a a i o i i i 8 non-apmklng speaking typaa of wrballatbna key: types of verbalizations 1. questioning 2. attention directing 3. answering 4. requesting 5. imitating 6. naming 7. negating 8. affirming 9. greeting 10. self-repetition 11. informative 12. uncodable figure 6: the types of verbalizations directed at the speaking and non-speaking children during perceptual classes. figure 7: the types of verbalizations directed at the speaking an non-speaking children during scholastic classes. table 4: the number of times that each type of verbalization was directed at group-1 (non-speaking children), over a period of five weeks. types of verbalizations week 1 week 2 week 3 week 4 week 5 total 1. questioning 1 3 2 4 1 11 2. attention directing 3 2 1 2 8 1 3. answering o : 4. requesting 4 2 1 7 1 5. imitating ο i 6. naming 1 1 1 2 7. negating 1 1 8. affirming 1 1 1 3 9. greeting 0 10. self-repetition / 0 11. informative 1 3 4 12. uncodable 0 y 13.total 6 11 5 11 3 36 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) interaction between teacher and the non-speaking as well as speaking children in the classroom 39 o f q u a n t i t y and quality. one can conclude that two main trends have been identified in the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children, namely she directed only a small amount of interaction at the non-speaking children and that she only used a limited variety of utterances when in interaction with the non-speaking children. it has, however, become apparent that the children's verbal abilities were not the only factor which influenced . the findings mentioned above, but certainly had a larger impact than the other factors. conclusion the initial hypothesis of the study, that the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children would differ from the interaction with the speaking children in terms of quantity and quality (cicognani & zani, 1992) was proven to be correct. it has also become clear, however, that although the children's verbal ability had the greatest influence on the teacher's interaction, other factors might also have played a role. additional factors were identified as important in determining the quantity and quality of the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking children, namely, the teacher's attitude towards the nonspeaking children, the teacher's knowledge of the importance of inclusion into interaction for the non-speaking children and the teacher's skill at including the non-speaking children in interaction (beukelman & mirenda, 1992). other important factors are the teacher's expectations of the non-speaking children and the ability to accurately predict whether a non-speaking child would be capable of completing a task (light & mcnaughton, 1993). the nonspeaking child's personality also has a significant influence on the teacher's interactions (beveridge et al., 1989). a teacher's skill in involving the non-speaking child in interaction has been found to influence the child's social development and learning as well as the child's level of spontaneous participation (kelly, 1978; mirenda & donnellan, 1986). in order to change attitudes, increase knowledge and improve skills, in-service teacher training programmes could be implemented (blackstone, 1989). training should provide teachers with a way to monitor and modify classroom interactions, when dealing with the non-speaking child, to promote participation, development and learning (loeding, za'ngari & lloyd, 1990). the teachers should be involved in; the process of deciding the content of the training programme (bottorf & depape, 1982). although the teacher's interaction with the non-speaking child is an important consideration in classroom intervention, the teacher's interaction is influenced by the child's participation (mirenda & donnellan, 1986). intervention with the child is, therefore, equally as important as intervention with the teacher. research has found that the child's role as an active participant in communication is an important factor in determining whether the interaction had been meaningful (mirenda & donnellan, 1986). although verbal interaction was the primary mode for learning, and it was, therefore, a valid variable to consider in the study, a shortcoming of this study is that it ignored the possible impact of natural gestures and nonspeaking communication on the child. however, natural gestures as a communication system have been found to be a very limited system, in terms of the small vocabulary and the low level of abstraction (musselwhite & st. louis, 1989). one could hypothesise that the use of natural gestures in interaction could, at best, have had a slight positive impact on the child's communication. however, further research should be done to determine the proportion and types of non-speaking interaction between the teacher and the two groups of children to determine the nature and impact thereof on the non-speaking child. research should also investigate the teacher's interaction with nonspeaking children who have aac systems as well as with non-speaking children who do not have aac systems, in order to determine the impact of the aac system on interaction. references basil, c. (1992). social interaction and learned helplessness in severely disabled children. augmentative and alternative communication, vol. 8:3, ppl88-199. baumgart,d., johnson,j. & helmsetter.e. (1990). augmentative and alternative communication systems for persons with moderate and severe disabilities. paul h. brookes publishing co. : baltimore. beukelman, d.r. (1991). magic and cost of communication competence. augmentative and alternative communication, vol. 7:1, pp2-10. beukelman, d. & mirenda, p. (1992). augmentative and alternative communication: management of severe communication disorders in children and adults. paul h. brookes publishing co.: baltimore. beukelman, d.r. & yorkston, k.m. (1982). communication interaction of adult communication augmentation system use. topics in language disorders, vol. 2, pp39-53. beveridge, m. & hurrel, p. (1980). teachers' responses to the initiations of esn(s) children. journal of child psychology and psychiatry, vol. 21, ppl75 181. beveridge, m., ramsden, g. & leudar, i. (1989). language and communication in mentally handicapped people. chapman and hall ltd.: london. blackstone, s. (1989). augmentative communication services in the schools. asha, vol.31, pp61 63. bottorf, l. & depape, d. (1982). initiating communication systems for severely speech-impaired persons. topic in language disorders, vol.2, 55 71. burd, l., hannes, k , bornhoeft, d. & fisher, w. (1988). anorth dakota prevalence study of non-speaking school-age children. language, speech and hearing services in schools, vol. 19:1, pp371-379. calculator, s. & luchko, c. (1983). evaluating the effectiveness of a communication board training program. journal of speech and hearing disorders, vol. 48, ppl85-191. cicognani, e. & zani, b. (1992). teacher-children interactions in a nursery school: an exploratory study. language and education, vol. 6:1, ppl-12. dalton, b. & bedrosian, j. (1989). communicative performance of adolescents with severe speech impairment: influence of context. journal of speech and hearing disorders, vol. 54, pp403-418. dunn, l. & dunn, l. (1981). peabody picture vocabulary test revised. american guidance service: minnesota. gardner, m. (1979). expressive one-word picture vocabulary test. academic therapy publications: california. grayshon, m. (1977). towards a social grammar of language. mouton publishers: the hague. harris, d. (1982). communicative interaction processes involving nonvocal physically handicapped children. topics in language disorders, vol. 22, pp21-36. hills, p. (1986). teaching and communication. croom helm: sydney. kelly, a. (1978). mixed-ability grouping. harper and row: london. klein, m. & harris, k. (1986). classroom communication functions of four learning-handicapped students. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, vol. 17, pp318-320. light, j. & mcnaughton, d. (1993). literacy and augmentative and alternative communication (aac): the expectations and priorities of parents and teachers. topics in language disorders, vol. 13:2, pp33-45. loeding, l., zangari, c., & lloyd, l. (1990). a "working party" die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 elsa popich & erna alant approach to planning in-service training in manual signs for an entire public school staff. augmentative and alternative communication, vol. 6:1 pp38-47. lossing, c., yorkston, k., & beukelman, d. (1985). communication augmentation systems: quantification in a natural setting. archives of physical medicine and rehabilitation, vol. 66, pp380-383. louw, d. (1990). menslike ontwikkeling.haum-tersier: pretoria. malamah-thomas, a. (1988). classroom interaction. oxford university press: london. matas, j., mathy-laikko, p., beukelman, d. & legresley, k. (1985). identifying the nonspeaking population: a demographic study. augmentative and alternative communication, vol. 1:1, ppl7-27. mirenda, p. & donnellan, a. (1986). effects of adult interaction style on conversational behaviour in students with severe communication problems. language, speech and hearing services in schools, vol. 17:2, ppl26-139. musselwhite, c. & st. louis, k. (1989). communication programming for persons with severe handicaps: vocal and augmentative strategies. college-hill press: boston. romski, m., sevcik, r., reumann, r. & pate, j. (1989). youngsters with moderate or severe mental retardation and severe spoken language impairments 1: extant communicative patterns journal of speech and hearing disorders, vol. 54, pp366-372 stuart, s., vanderhoof, d. & beukelman, d.r. (1993). topic and vocabulary use patterns of elderly woman. augmentative and alternative communication, vol. 9:2, pp95-107. wium, a. & alant, e. (1993). communication assessment schedule for severely handicapped students. unpublished study, department of communication pathology, university of pretoria. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental aphasia j o n e i s e n s o n , ph.d.* as recently as ten years ago, a paper on developmental aphasia would probably have been introduced with arguments either for or against the existence of the syndrome. although this line of argument may still be heard, most specialists in language disorders of children are more likely to be concerned about the differential symptoms of the syndrome rather than to question its existence, in the broadest possible sense, developmental aphasia may be considered to exist when we can establish that a neurologically handicapped child has failed to establish, or has severe retardation in, the understanding and production of language. in a narrower sense, and in the sense to which we shall address ourselves to the question, we consider that developmental aphasia is relatively specific to language function (absence or severe retardation of this function) and that the impairment cannot be ascribed to one or more of the frequent causes for language impairment. these causes include deafness, mental deficiency, motor disability involving the speech mechanism, or severe personality (emotional) problems. ft is not our intention in this paper to consider the question of differential diagnosis in any detail. fortunately, articles by a. l. benton1 and j. eisenson0 cover the question of the identification and differential diagnosis of the aphasic child. in this paper, therefore, we shall emphasize the possible etiology and the perceptual characteristics and intellectual functioning of the child designated as developmentally aphasic. etiology in the article by benton,1 referred to above, the author postulated two possible types of etiology for developmental aphasia. the first was an underlying impairment for associations or connections in the cerebral system between sensed sound and other cerebral processes through which meanings may be derived. the second postulation stressed the likelihood of the presence of defective perceptual processes which underlie the failure for normal language development. it is often possible for psychologists to demonstrate the second without convincing neurologists about the existence of the first. if, however, * professor, speech and h e a r i n g sciences, school of medicine. stanford university. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. , des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 6 jon eisenson we take the position that the psychologist, in assessing perceptual functions is really engaged in an extended neurological, then establishing perceptual impairment or dysfunctioning implies that the cerebral mechanisms are defective. this is so even though a routine neurological does not provide evidence for the other positive findings. this writer accepts the position that developmentally aphasic* children suffer from a primary impairment in those aspects of auditory perception necessary for language to be learned and produced. the impairment in auditory perception may be associated with actual brain damage incurred before birth, during birth, or during the first year to year and a half of life, or because of a delay in cerebral maturation which implicates the auditory centres. until fairly recently, clinical impression rather than the results of systematic experimental studies provided the data for our observations in regard to the auditory functioning of aphasic children. some of the more frequent clinical observations cited in the literature include difficulty in the localization of a source of sound, inconsistency of responses to sound, especially to speech sounds, marked oscillation of auditory threshold, inattention to auditory stimuli, and quick dissipation of attention (benton2). the acquisition of language by the end of the second year, most children indicate by their behaviour that they understand much of what is said to them by normal speaking adults. in addition, the children are well on their way to speaking much like the adults in their environment, but with enough variation to be speaking for themselves, as well as expressing their special selves as unique members of their environment. by the end of the third year, most children understand not only what is said to them, but are able, instantly, to comprehend an amazingly large number of verbal formulations to which they have had no previous exposure. thus, we may conclude that normal children are able to listen and understand creatively, and are innovative and creative in their own utterances. each time a child arranges (formulates) a number of words he has not before so uttered, he is demonstrating creativity in verbal behaviour. chomsky,4 in an article on language and the mind, sums up this position as follows: the fact surely is . . . that the n u m b e r of sentences in one's native language that one will immediately understand, with no feeling of difficulty or strange* hereafter, the single terms aphasic, of aphasia, will be used rather than developmental aphasic, or developmental aphasia. it should be understood that a child who has established language may, as in the case of an adult, become aphasic as a result of brain damage. t h ' s would constitute acquired aphasia in a child. we are not, however, concerned with this problem in this paper. journal of the south african logopedie society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental aphasia 17 ness, is astronomical, and that the n u m b e r of patterns that underlie our normal use of language and that correspond to meaningful and easily comprehensible sentences in our language is orders of magnitude greater than the number of seconds in a lifetime, or the number of seconds in the history of the language for that matter. it is in this sense that the normal use of language is innovative, in fact, potentially infinite in variety. in the normal acquisition of language a child somehow learns to listen so as to discriminate the sounds and combinations of sounds in the linguistic system of his environment, to isolate some words, and to produce utterances consisting of combinations of words according to a set of rules (the grammar) of the language cr languages to which he is exposed. interestingly, though the normal child seems to show awareness of the basic sound units (phonemes) of a linguistic system during the first year of his life, he does not usually become completely proficient in his articulatory ability until he is seven or eight years of age. along with phonemic and articulatory proficiency, the normal child also learns the melody or intonation of his language. by age eight, perhaps somewhat earlier for girls and somewhat later for some boys, except for vocal pitch range, most normal children speak essentially the way they will as adults. individually, of course, some children will develop larger comprehension and more productive vocabularies than others, and some will be able to understand and produce more complex grammatically correct utterances than others. vocal nuances may be better appreciated and productively controlled by some children than by others, so that irony, sarcasm, and the implications of utterances that are conveyed through subtle changes of inflection become individualized acquisitions. such acquisitions, along with the development of vocabularies, may continue indefinitely through the life of the speaker. before considering the specific nature of the perceptual impairments that we consider to underlie the failure for language acquisition in the aphasic child, four brief hypotheses will be stated in positive form in regard to the normal establishment of verbal behaviour. (1) as far as we presently know, only human beings are capable of learning to use language without being stimulus-bound to the events, or replication of the events, that were initially associated with and evoked the original linguistic products. (2) no theory of learning at present adequately explains the acquisition of language beyond the utterance of single words to identify objective events. learning theorists as of now, are unable to explain how a child can understand verbal formulations never responded to before, or to produce acceptable verbal formulations never before tried by the speaker. (3) concepts of imprinting and readiness are needed to explain the critical period — between fifteen and thirty months of age — tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 jon eisenson when most normal children establish verbal behaviour. in lower animals behaviour imprinting is considered to be species specific. imprinting is presumably related to special sensitivities and response potentialities which enable a member of a species to establish new behaviour patterns with a minimum of exposure, effort and opportunity for practice. (4) the critical period for most normal children for the establishment of verbal behaviour is between fifteen and thirty months. comprehension of language as manifested by appropriate non-verbal responses is normally established between nine to twelve months.* medical and psychological findings aphasic children vary considerably from one another in regard to objective findings resulting from medical, neurological and psychological assessment. as a total population, however, they are different from other non-verbal children who assess as being primarily mentally retarded or severely hard-of-hearing. the findings that will be summarized are based on examinations of more than two hundred children, of whom seventy-three were designated as aphasic. the examinations were conducted at the institute for childhood aphasia, school of medicine, stanford university. electroencephalogram findings. thirty-six of the group were found to have positive electroencephalograms. twenty-two showed localized abnormalities, of which nineteen were in the left hemisphere. these findings are in general accord with those of goldstein, landau, and kleffner10 who report that forty percent of sixty-nine aphasic children showed abnormal electroencephalograms. although these investigators found about the same percentage of abnormalities in their comparison population of one hundred and fourteen deaf children, the aphasics had a higher incidence of focal abnormalities (14.5%) than did the deaf children (6.1%). audiological examination. the aphasic child often gives the. impression of being either hard-of-hearing or deaf. objective findings indicate that many, perhaps thirty per cent, do in fact have mild to moderate hearing losses based on results of objective audiometry. however, a typical audiological report is likely to indicate that the amount of hearing loss based on test findings is not sufficient to explain the severity of the language impairment. functionally, the i 1 * f o r a detailed consideration of the implications of these basic concepts in regard to normal language development see ε. h. lenneberg. 1 2 journal of the south african logopedi society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental aphasia 19 aphasic child with a hearing loss shows impairment for listening rather than for the physical reception of sound per se. he is likely to show more impairment for listening to human speech than to animal or environmental noises. in this sense, he appears to have a selective hearing loss. the reasons for this will be considered later in our discussion of the aphasic child's perceptual dysfunctions. intellectual functioning. a basic assumption in regard to most aphasic children is that despite their normal intellectual potential they are intellectually inefficient. the implication of inefficiency is that the child's performance tends to break down under conditions of noise, stress, and awareness of error, more readily than we would expect for a normal child. performance of aphasic children on standardized tests is characterized by variability. so is performance on learning tasks. a given child's productions for the same task (test item or learning situation) may vary from complete failure at one time to a high degree of success at another. if we assume that a child's best performance is indicative of his intellectual potential, then we would conclude that most aphasic children approximate the norms and the learning capabilities of their peers, at least when the estimates are based on non-verbal situations. functionally and practically, however, most aphasic children tend to perform below the level of their best efforts. behavioural observations of aphasic children when they are involved with difficult test items or difficult learning situations include strong manifestations of perseveration, expressions of hostility directed to the examiner or to the materials at hand, and often considerable hyperactivity. some children, however, withdraw from continuing with the test or learning tasks rather than act out against the situation. a highly significant clinical observation of test and learning performance of aphasic children is their tendency to lose sight of an underlying principle needed for the solution of a test task or problem. thus, if a test item requires that the child arrange a number of figures or cards in an alternating series such as a circle and a cross, a child may arrange half the figures in the required order, and then place the remainder in a random order. a related characteristic performance error is failure to carry over a principle from one test item to another. thus, even if an aphasic child succeeds in a task — e.g. pointing to the different picture on the columbia mental maturity scale, the examiner cannot assume that the child will know what he has to do on succeeding items. often it seems that each item is a task unto itself, that the aphasic child has to work out anew, or be reminded by the clinician, that tasks b, c, etc., are but items in a series that will be solved by the application of the same principle employed for task a. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 jon eisenson impairments of auditory perception associated with developmental aphasia although some aphasic children may have related productive problems (dysarthria and oral apraxia), we believe that the underlying problem is one of defective auditory perception. as noted earlier, some aphasic children have objectively determined hearing loss, and most aphasic children are functionally considerably more impaired than the degree of hearing loss would suggest. beyond the implications of such hearing limitation, aphasic children as a total population are more severely impaired in auditory perception — in their ability to discriminate and process auditorily those events that constitute speech — and in their ability to store, retrieve, and derive meaning from oral signals and symbols. in the discussion that follows we will consider some possible bases for these impairments. defective capacity for storing οϊ speech signals. we have been able to demonstrate that aphasic children, despite many initial errors, can be trained to discriminate and match isolated speech sounds when discriminations and matchings are based on immediate recall (mcreynolds13' '"). however, we have also observed that most aphasic children make considerably more matching errors than do their peers when a period of delay is introduced before an opportunity for matching. our assumption is that the children are not able to store and retain the signals after short periods of delay. in contrast, performance involving responses to mechanical sounds is usually retained. a possible explanation for this observation is that speech signals call for different storage and control than do other kinds of auditory events. support for this assumption is provided by the results of several studies by members of the haskins laboratories. dr. liberman and his associates,1'1 based upon their investigations, state: the conclusion that there is a speech mode, and that it is characterized by processes different from those underlying the perception of other sounds, is strengthened by recent indications that speech and nonspeech sounds are processed primarily in different hemispheres of the brain. impairment of sound generalization in contextual utterance. in the immediately preceding paragraphs we considered the assumption that aphasic children have an impairment in the discrimination ,οΐ isolated speech signals because of storage-retrieval dysfunction. aysecond and related impairment is concerned with the nature of speech signals in contextual utterance. except for one-phoneme words, such as / and a, our speech tokens consist of combinations of consonants and vowels, or consonant clusters and vowels. in spoken utterance, individual sounds are modified according to their contextual environment. thus, the t of too is somewhat different from the t of pit, or those in hit it and hit that. despite these differences, normal children learn to i journal of the south african logopedi society, vol. 16, no. , dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental aphasia 21 recognize all of these variants of t as essentially the same sound, or at least as having essential similarities more significant than their incidental differences. we know, of course, that phoneticians refer to the basic sound category as a phoneme, and the variants as allophones. the capacity for generalization as to speech sound (phonemic) categories we believe to be impaired in aphasic children. if this assumption is correct, then it is likely that aphasic children perceive speech sounds as discrete auditory events. because no two speakers are precisely alike in their articulatory products,* the child's storage capacity for auditory events is both overtaxed and lacking in a matching to sample system to permit him to determine what auditory event he is responding to at any given time. in brief, the child is without functional basic categories for the processing of speech events. impairments related to sequencing (rate and order of utterance). aphasic children may lack the capacity for listening as rapidly as necessary to perceive and process speech. william hardy7 refers to the positive ability of normal children to process speech input as auding which he defines as the integrative functions in the brain's management of acoustic information. auding involves such related functions as the ability to discriminate between sounds on the bases of differences in intensity, frequency, and duration as well as the rate at which the changes themselves occur. unless a listener can do this, he cannot distinguish between words such as ask and ax, fits and fist, or understand such statements as the household pets became pests, or jane bit her fists when she had fits. how rapidly must a child be able to listen to be competent in auding? in broad terms, the answer is rapidly enough to make matchings between ongoing auditory events and those events, or residuals of events, that have been stored somewhere in his nervous system. he must also be able to keep in mind the order of events and be competent in making temporal resolutions. normally, all of this can be achieved in a small fraction of a second, a matter of milliseconds. experimental evidence on both normal and brain-damaged subjects provides us with information as to the minimum interval of time necessary for discrimination (resolutions) to be made between successive signals, the interval necessary for both resolution and temporal order judgment, and the effects of experience in modifying (reducing) minimum time interval between signals for the required * actually, no speaker's articulations are precisely the same even for repeated utterances. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 jon eisenson judgments. broadbent and ladefoged3 report that the time required for a correct time order judgment between pip — hiss and hiss — pip was reduced from one hundred and fifty milliseconds to thirty milliseconds after repeated trials on this task. hirsch and sherrick9 found that an experienced subject required an interval of twenty milliseconds to make correct judgments of the presented order of two events — a light and a sound signal — when these events are produced repeatedly in the same order. when such a judgment has to be made on the basis of a single presentation, naive subjects required about sixty milliseconds for the same percentage (75%) of accuracy of judgment (hirsch, i. j., and fraisse, p.8) the experiments cited above were performed with normal subjects. subjects with cerebral pathology required an appreciably longer interval of time for temporal order judgment tasks. efrons found that some aphasic adults required as much as a full second to make correct judgment as to the order of two ten millisecond pulses markedly different in frequency, whereas neurologically normal adults performed this task in approximately fifty-sixty milliseconds. there are, unfortunately, few investigations with children as subjects. the findings, generally, are along the same lines as for adults. generally, children with aphasic involvements require considerably more time than do normal children to make correct judgments as to the timeorder of events. lowe and campbell" found that a group of aphasoid children, ranging in age from seven to fourteen years, needed a mean time of three hundred and fifty-seven milliseconds for time-order judgments (range from fifty-five to seven hundred milliseconds) compared with a mean time of thirty-six point one milliseconds (range from fifty to eighty milliseconds) for normal control subjects. the specific task involved required the subject to indicate the correct order between two fifteen millisecond sound pulses, one at 2200 cycles per second and one at 400 cycles per second. impairments of sequencing and aphasic dysfunction. both clinical observations and psychodiagnostic test findings tend to support the impression that aphasic children are impaired in their ability to sequence speech events. for some aphasic children, the impairment may be more general and involve the processing of any series (sequence) of events in time and/or space. the impairment is, usually less severe for visual events, in part because most visual ̂ events are relatively static so that it is possible;for a child to look again at and so confirm or correct an impression. it is ordinarily not possible to listen again to a succession of auditory events. words once spoken, or noises once produced, are ephemeral. they can be reproduced only by retrieval from memory. j ι ι regardless of the manner of production, or modality intake, all linguistic events are temporal and sequential. we cannot understand journal of the south african logopedi society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental aphasia 23 language of more than a single sound or a single written signal — and there is very little to be understood with such a limitation — unless we are able to process ongoing events in the light of immediately past events and with anticipation of events about to occur. each moment of linguistic experience provides the listener (speaker) with an opportunity as well as a need for confirmation or rejection of preceding perceptions and assumptions. the aphasic child is seriously impaired in this game of perceptual and conceptual probabilities. some possible reasons for this impairment were suggested earlier in our speculations about poor storage capacity for auditory events, and the difficulty related to weaknesses in phonemic and linguistic generalization. it is also likely that the aphasic child has an impairment for the processing of sequences of speech events. summary and implications if our observations are correct and our speculations tenable, we may regard the aphasic child as one who may be defective in: (a) storage and retrieval of sounds; (b) in phonemic generalization; (c) in sequencing; and (d) more generally, and more broadly psychologically, in ability to generalize and to apply principles to situations that share a critical and determining common feature. the aphasic child may be born structurally ill-equipped for the acquisition of verbal behaviour. it would be helpful if at this point, we could indicate with confidence the requisite capacities and the functional structures which enable all but a small percentage of children to begin to speak and to develop verbal behaviour according to the expectations of the concerned members of their environment. we may speculate but we are by no means certain as to how a child can understand verbal formulations he has never heard before, and to produce his own formulations with considerable confidence that what he says will be understood by others. we assume, of course, that normal hearing acuity, normal perceptual ability, normal sequencing, and a fair amount of intellect are required for the acquisition of language. in regard to intellect, it is important to appreciate that most children who are mentally subnormal, unless the subnormality is profound, nevertheless learn to speak. yet some children who indicate through non-verbal behaviour that they have adequate intelligence, that is, they perform about as expected in situations where verbal mediation is not required — fail to acquire language without direct therapeutic intervention. the aphasic and some autistic children are among those tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 jon eisenson with adequate intelligence who do not learn to acquire language spontaneously. some clinicians and not a few linguists take recourse to a philosophic attitude about what the capacities and structures might be for a child to acquire speech. the position they take is that a child learns to speak because speech is a human species-specific function. so, according to lenneberg,11 the development of language, also a species-specific phenomenon, is related physiologically, structurally, and dcvelopmentally to the other two typically human characteristics, cerebral dominance and maturational history. language is not an arbitrarily adopted behaviour, facilitated by accidentally fortunate anatomical arrangements in the oral cavity and larynx, but an activity that develops harmoniously by neccssary integration of neuronal and skeletal structures and by reciprocal adaptation of various physiological processes. we do not pretend that all or even most of the evidence needed to explain the lack or severe delay of speech in aphasic children is presently available. what evidence we do have strongly suggests to us that aphasic children are lacking in the basic capacities and in the correlative abilities and integrations necessary for normal language acquisition. perhaps these children are not pre-wired neurologically as well as they should be to integrate what they need, to be proficient receivers and senders of sound signals. perhaps aphasic children have a slower central nervous system maturation than normal children or even our mentally subnormal children who acquire speech. it is likely that some aphasic children develop perceptual defences because of demands made on their systems which are beyond their capacities at critical times. these are some, but not all of the possibilities which must be considered if we are to understand the nature of the problem of developmental aphasia, and if we are to develop rational and significant therapeutic and training procedures. opsomming die afatiese kind kan beskou word as defektief in die volgende funksies: (a) opberging en herwinning van klanke; (b) fonemiese veralgemening; ^ (c) opeenvolging; 7 (d) die vermoe om te veralgemeen en beginsels toe te pas op situasies wat 'n kritieke en bepalende algemene faktor deel. hierdie swakhede bemoeilik die aanleer van taal vir so 'n kind; verder kan hy ook gebore wees met 'n strukturele onvermoe om verbale gedrag aan te leer. journal of the south african logopedie society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental aphasia 25 adekwate intelligensie tenspyt, leer die afatiese kind, soos die outistiese kind, nie taal spontaan aan nie. die subnormale kind daarenteen, leer uiteindelik taal aan mits die subnormaliteit nie te groot is nie. alhoewel ons nie oor al die getuienis beskik om die vertraagde spraak in afatiese kinders te verklaar nie, blyk dit tog dat hierdie kinders nie oor die nodige korrelatiewe vermoens beskik om taal aan te leer nie. alle moontlikhede moet in gedagte gehou word by die beskouing van die aard van die probleem van ontwikkelingsafasie. references 1. benton, a. l. (1964): developmental aphasia and brain damage. cortex /, 40-52. 2. benton, a. l. (1967): problems of test construction in the field of aphasia. cortex 111, 32-58. 3. broadbent, d. e. and ladefoged, p. (1959): auditory perception of temporal order. journal of the acoustical society of america, 31, 15-49. 4. chomsky, n. (1968): language and the mind, i. columbia university f o r u m 5-10. 5. efron. r. (1963): temporal perception, aphasia, and deja vu. brain, 86, 403-424. 6. eisenson, j. (1966): developmental patterns of non-verbal children. journal of neurological science 3, 313-320. 7. hardy, w. g. (1965): on language disorders in young children. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 1, 3-16. 8. hirsh, i. j. and fraisse, p. (1965): central institute for the deaf, periodic progress reports. 9. hirsh, i. j. and sherrick (1965): perceived order in different sense modalities. journal of experimental psychology, 62, 423-432. 10. goldstein, r., landau, w. m. and kleffner, f. r. (1968): neurological assessment of deaf and aphasic children. transactions of the american otologic society, 122-136. 11. lenneberg, ε. h. (1966): the natural history of language. smith, f. and miller, g. t h e genesis of language, m.i.t. press, 219-252. 12. lenneberg, ε. h. (1967): biological foundations of language, chapter 4. wiley, new york. 13. liberman, a. m., cooper, f. s., shankweiler, d. p. and studdert-kennedy, m. (1967): perception of the speech code. psychological review, 74, 431-461. 14. lowe, d. a. and campbell, r. a. (1965): temporal discrimination in aphasoid and normal children. journal of speech and hearing research. 8, 313-314. 15. mcreynolds, l. k. (1964): operant conditioning discrimination in aphasic children. unpublished ph.d. dissertation, stanford university. 16. mcreynolds, l. k. (1966): operant conditioning for investigating speech sound discrimination in aphasic children. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 9, 519-528. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. , des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 5 3 nonspeaking children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater pretoria area: implications for speech-language therapists juan bornman and erna alant centre for augmentative and alternative communication department of communication pathology university of pretoria abstract the aim of this study was to describe children enrolled in registered schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater pretoria area in order to compile a profile of nonspeaking children. emphasis was placed on the prevalence of nonspeaking children as well as on their functioning in different skill areas in order to assess the need for service delivery. two questionnaires were developed; the first for obtaining biographical data from teachers; the second for obtaining information on the communication and related abilities of children between 3 -12 years. the particular teachers completed the questionnaires in conjunction with fieldworkers. results indicated a high prevalence (38%) of nonspeaking children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the pretoria area and also indicated that they were a heterogeneous group regarding communication and related abilities. this survey was the first step in determining prevalence and describing nonspeaking children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in south africa. results also indicated that these children are in great need of additional service delivery with special reference to augmentative and alternative communication (aac) strategy implementation. opsomming die doel van hierdie studie was om kinders ingeskryf by geregistreerde skole vir verstandelik erg gestremde kinders in die groter pretoria-area te beskryf om sodoende 'n profiel van nie-sprekende kinders saam te stel. klem is geplaas op die voorkoms van nie-sprekende kinders asook op hulle funksionering met betrekking tot verskillende vaardigheidsareas om sodoende die behoefte aan dienslewering te bepaal. twee vraelyste is ontwikkel; die eerste vir die verkryging van persoonlike inligting van die betrokke onderwysers en die tweede vir die verkryging van inligting rakende die kommunikasieen verwante vaardighede van kinders tussen die ouderdomme 3 -12 jaar. die betrokke onderwysers het hierdie vraelyste in samewerking met veldwerkers voltooi. resultate het 'n hoe voorkoms (38%) van nie-sprekende kinders in skole vir verstandelik erg gestremde kinders in die pretoria-area aangetoon, asook dat hulle 'n heterogene groep is met betrekking tot kommunikasieen verwante vaardighede. hierdie studie was die eerste stap in die bepaling van die voorkoms en beskrywing van niesprekende kinders in skole vir verstandelik erg gestremde kinders in suid-afrika. resultate het ook aangetoon dat hierdie kinders 'n groot behoefte het aan addisionele dienslewering, met spesifieke verwysing na aanvullende en alternatiewe kommunikasie (aak) strategie-implementasie. ι keywords: augmentative and alternative communication, nonspeaking children, service delivery, prevalence. introduction the devastating effect that the inability to speak has on an individual's daily life has been described extensively during the last two decades by nonspeaking people themselves as well as by those who interact closely with them. not only does the inability to speak make it more difficult to convey messages and to participate in interactions, it also impacts on language and the development of literacy skills. kopenhaver and yoder (1992) pointed out that as many as 80% of nonspeaking people have very poor literacy skills. furthermore, they stress that poor literacy skills in this group of people relate not only to the inabil!ty of the individual to speak, but also to a lack of exposure to literacy experiences. as being nonspeaking is often interpreted by people as an inability to think and learn, these nonspeaking children are frequently denied learning opportunities. being nonspeaking within a speaking world therefore creates a number of attitudinal barriers that have to be overcome. "speech is the most important thing we have. it makes us a person and not a thing. no one should ever have to be a "thing" " (joseph, 1986). it is against this background that two major issues can be identified in the augmentative and alternative communication (aac) literature. the first relates to the importance of identifying children who are nonspeaking or at risk not to develop speech, and the second focuses on the different ways in which people who are not able to express die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43,1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 themselves adequately through speech can gain access to other means of communication to prevent the isolation and frustration which is associated with the inability to express oneself. various studies have been conducted internationally to establish the prevalence of nonspeaking children within the special school context. different definitions were used to define nonspeaking, e.g. "persons who have a severe speech problem due to physical, neuromuscular, cognitive or emotional deficits and not due primarily to hearing impairment" (matas, mathy-laikko, beukelman and legresley 1985). bloomberg and johnson (1990) expanded on this definition by defining the nonspeaking population as "persons who are temporarily or permanently unable to meet all their communication needs verbally". the north dakota survey, however, aimed at quantifying the term nonspeaking by defining it as "people who use 15 or less intelligible words" (burd, hammes, bornhoeft and fisher, 1988). this cut-off point was utilized in this study on the assumption that with normal development, speech becomes a functional means of communication towards the end of the single word stage, when a typical expressive vocabulary consists of 15 20 words (cantwell and baker, 1985). it was further decided to use the term "nonspeaking" as opposed to "nonverbal" as the latter is an ambiguous term which technically means "without language", and is thus not used for describing individuals, but rather for describing evaluation tools (e.g., nonverbal intelligence of individuals who have difficulty comprehending or producing spoken language) (lloyd, fuller and arvidson, 1996). the north dakota survey reported that 2,4% of children in special schools in north dakota were nonspeaking (burd, et al., 1988). it further indicated a strong decrease (almost 50%) in the prevalence of non-speech after three years of schooling. matas et al., 1985, conducted a similar survey in washington state's special schools and included urban and rural populations. results from this demographic study indicated that 3,6% children of the urban and 6% children of the rural special schools were nonspeaking. in another study, which was conducted in germany and which included the whole school population, it was reported that 0,35% of all school children were nonspeaking (coon, kremer and hildebrand, 1992). when these demographic surveys were extended to include the total population, a prevalence rate of 0,12% was found in a study conducted in australia (bloomberg etal., 1990). it is interesting to note that all these studies were conducted in western countries. no similar studies have been conducted in developing countries. as far as nonspeaking people's access to aac systems is concerned, the washington survey reported that at least 18% of children in special education used some type of aac system, whilst 23,3% were being trained to use an aac system (matas et al., 1985). the other 29,2% were using only unintelligible speech and although the rest of the children met the criteria for using an aac system, they were in need of initial or follow-up evaluations. in the german survey it was reported that the majority of children used unaided communication systems (facial expressions, followed by gestures, touching, and pointing respectively) (coon et al., 1992). these results are further supported by results from the washington survey which also reported that nonspeaking children use gestures and emotional reactions (e.g., facial expressions and gross body the so juan bomman and erna alant movements) as the predominant communication system (matas et al., 1985). this tendency also seems to be present in older children, as a survey amongst university students also found gestures to be the most frequently used system (huer, 1991). as previously mentioned no studies have as yet been conducted in africa on the prevalence of nonspeaking children. this lack of descriptive statistics provided the major impetus for this survey. in order to assist professionals in planning intervention for nonspeaking children, a profile of the nonspeaking population in south africa is needed. it is against this background that the present study aimed at describing the communication abilities of children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities. method aim the main aim of this study was to establish the prevalence of nonspeaking children enrolled in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater pretoria metropolitan substructure and by compiling a profile regarding their functioning in different skill areas (cognitive, motor, sensory, communication and social). in addition, their exposure to intervention was also described. research design in order to realise the aims of the study an analytical (quantitative) survey design was used as this type of design is particularly useful in describing demographic information within a particular context (groenewald, 1986). subjects description of schools all registered schools for children with severe mental disabilities within a particular geographical area, namely the greater pretoria metropolitan substructure (including pretoria central, eersterust, laudium, atteridgeville and mamelodi) were included. criteria for inclusion in the different schools varied, although certain broad principles remained the same, i.e. an iq of between 30 60 (measured as reliable as possible), age between 3 18 years at admission, a physical ability to move about and an ability at admission to show communication potential. these schools were selected as the candidacy for the use of aac strategies and techniques is high within these schools and they consequently form one of the primary focus areas in the aac field (matas et al., 1985). these schools all had to be registered with one of the following departments prior to 1995 : department of education and training, department of education and culture or the department of health. no informal schools were included. schools for the deaf and schools for the blind were also excluded as they were traditionally not the main area of focus when using aac strategies (coon etal., 1992 and bloomberg etal., 1990). this brought the total number of schools to 10. more detailed information on these schools is presented in table 1. according to table 1 there was a broad variation in the schools. some schools had a large number of pupils (school no 6), whilst others had relatively few (school no 2). some schools also had assistance from therapists (schools no 1 and 9) whilst others had no therapists (schools no 8 and african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) n e a k i n g children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater p r e t o r i a area: implications for speech-language therapists 10) all the schools had class aids to assist the teachers. 2-page questionnaire that was completed by a fieldworker description of teachers teachers acted as the main informants for this survey, as various national (skuy, westaway, makula and perold, 1988) and international studies (matas, et al., 1985) confirmed that teachers can meaningfully describe pupils' functioning. it is thus important to provide a brief description of the teachers. information related to relevant biographical information regarding the teachers was obtained from questionnaire i (bornman, 1995). this was a short table 1 : description of schools included in the study 55 in conjunction with a teacher. the total number of teachers included in the survey was 55. this questionnaire revealed that 98% of teachers were female. table 2 indicates that the teachers were a heterogeneous group regarding qualifications, training and years of teaching experience. regarding qualifications, it was found that 82% of the teachers had post-matric training, whilst 18% had only school training varying from std 8 std 10. fifteen teachers reported that they had no special training for working with children with severe mental disabilities, whilst 40 teachers reported that they had some spe# area # o f pupils # o f teachers # o f class aids # o f speech therapists # o f ot's # o f physiotherapists τ i τ i # o f class aids # o f speech therapists # o f physiotherapists 1 pretoria central 115 53 12 6 12 2 2 3 2 pretoria central 18 11 3 3 2 0 0 1 twice a week 3 eersterust 47 23 5 4 2 1 twice a week 1 once a week 1 physio assistant 4 pretoria central 30 15 3 2 4 1 once a week 1 once a week 1 once a week 5 soshanguve 110 25 6 3 2 0 1 0 6 pretoria central 336 125 34 16 10 0 1 0 7 laudium 57 35 5 4 7 1 1 0 8 mamelodi 78 21 4 3 3 0 0 0 9 pretoria central 60 39 12 10 6 1 1 0 10 atteridgeville 120 65 11 4 4 0 0 0 total 971 412 95 55 legend: τ = total number at school; i = number included in survey i ι table 2: biographical information of teachers qualifications | less than 10 years) at school i 10 12 years at school 2 3 years after school more than 4 years after school η =6 11% η = 4 7% η = 39 71% η = 6 11% special training * in-service training: less than 2 years diploma: less than 1 year diploma: less than 2 years diploma: less than 3 years n = 15 27% η = 5 9% n = 10 18% n = 10 18% years of teaching experience at schools for children with mental disabilities less than 1 year 1 2 years 3 5 years more than 5 years η = 3 5% η = 3 5% n = 13 24% η = 36 66% * please note that 15 teachers had no special training, and thus the total η = 40. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43,1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 cial training, ranging from in-service training to special diplomas. the majority of teachers had less than 2 years of in-service training. of the 25 teachers with special diplomas, 5 had diplomas of less than 1 year and 10 had diplomas of less than 2 years and less than 3 years respectively. furthermore it was found that 36 of the teachers had more than 5 years teaching experience at schools for children with severe mental disabilities, whilst only 3 had less than 1 year experience. description of children information regarding the children was obtained from the teacher and recorded in questionnaire ii. a total number of 412 children was included as they met the set criteria, namely that their chronological ages had to be between 3,0 and 12,11 years (only children from the preprimary and primary school phase were included) and the children's names had to appear on the class list. when referring to the total number of children, it would amount to 412 children, and when referring to the nonspeaking children it would amount to 158 children. as previously mentioned, nonspeaking children were classified as children who spoke 15 words or less (burd et al., 1988). more detailed information on the children included in the survey follows in the "results" section. procedure material for the purpose of this study two questionnaires were developed, one for descriptive biographical information about the teacher; and the second for information about each individual child in the class. the contents of both questionnaires were based on those used in the international demographic surveys on the nonspeaking children (bloomberg et al, 1990; matas et al, 1985; coon et al, 1992 and burd et al, 1988). questionnaire i dealt with the biographical details of the teachers and covered aspects such as identifying data (name of school, number of children in the class, and gender), qualifications, years of experience, specialized training, and whether the teacher received any additional assistance from class aids, therapists or volunteers (bornman, 1995). questionnaire ii covered the children's background information (gender and birth date), information on the functioning in different skill areas (communication, cognition, motor, sensory and socioemotional), literacy skills (reading, writing and mathematical) as well as intervention. this information, as well as the particular 5 point rating scale that was used, is described in more detail in appendix a. data collection the data collection procedure required that both questionnaire i and questionnaire ii be tested in a pilot study, after which certain revisions were made. permission to conduct this survey was then obtained from the local school authorities and 5 fieldworkers were trained in completing both questionnaires effectively and efficiently by providing them with the necessary skills and knowledge. fieldworkers were placed individually at the 10 schools, and after the principal had introduced them to the various teachers and the aims of the survey had been exjuan bornman and erna a l a i l t plained, the fieldworkers started completing the question naires in conjunction with the teachers. as the fieldworker had face to face contact with the teachers, they clarifij additional questions that seemed confusing to individual teachers. a total number of 55 teachers and 412 children were included in the survey. all questionnaires were then encoded, and the data analyzed using a variety of statists cal procedures. analysis of data data was analyzed and statistically processed in order to provide quantitative results of the survey. statistical procedures included descriptive statistics (discrete frequency distribution counts and percentages) and logistic regression procedures (in order to determine which variable(s) were the best predictors of non-speech). these variables were selected by means of a stepwise selection procedure (proc logist of sas). loglinear correlation coefficients were also applied to the data (in order to determine the strength of the relationship(s) between variables). this procedure was conducted in the following way: firstly a chi-square test was applied to a table and if significant dependencies were found on the 5% level, the loglinear model was applied to measure the strength of the relationships between the variables. a critical value of ζ = >± 2,58 was used to judge whether a particular variable was significant. the p4f procedure of the bmpd statistical package was used for the loglinear analysis. results and discussion l b accurately describe the prevalence of nonspeaking children it is important to provide a comprehensive overview of all the children included in the survey. it is for this reason that all the figures include both the speaking (n=254) and nonspeaking (n=158) children. 1. background information from figure 1 it is clear that 38% of children between the ages 3,0 12,11 years in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater pretoria area were nonspeaking. as previously mentioned, "nonspeaking" referred to children "who use 15 or less intelligible words" the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 4, 199 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 57 . children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater n o o s p ? a ^ £ , a : implications for speech-language therapists p r e t o r x a ^ ^ i m p l i e s t h a t of the total number months were excluded. the majority of nonspeaking chil( b u r j e i a h i ' l d r e n included in the survey, 158 were of 4 1 / c , * „ the high prevalence figure (38%) of n ° n s p aking children was the most significant factor in n ° n s p e a a r c h this figure is significantly higher than those c o m p a r a b l e international surveys which reported nfotner 6 % (matas et al., 1985). the lower inci-o f ° t h e r e < o f 3 5 8 ^ rfnonspeaking children (3,5%) was reported in urd e n c e c h o o l s j a n d 6% in rural/remote schools implying that * f n s r e v a l e n c e of the present study might have been higher crural schools had been included (matas etal, 1985; aieu 0 when comparing speaking and nonspeaking children der the age of 6 included in the survey, it is clear that ""ore nonspeaking children are placed in schools for chil!γ θ η with severe mental disabilities than their speaking counterparts. however, at the age of 6, the number of speaking and nonspeaking children are almost even. the majority of nonspeaking children was 9 years old. the number of 13 year olds is very low, due to the fact that children older than 12 years and 6 months were rounded off to 13 years, and children older than 12 years and 11 50 b 40 20 46|·. 40 7 • 3? 4 42 34 -22 / 16 17,/ / 11 \ ^ v ' 7 8 9 age in years • nonspeaking (n= 158) speaking (n 254) figure 2 : ages of the children table 3: gender of children variable % boys % girls total group of children 66% 34% nonspeaking children 1 68% 32% dren was 9 years old, with a greater distribution at the ages higher than 9, indicating that more children were older, rather than younger than 9. however, before age 6 (when speaking and nonspeaking children are almost even) the survey indicated that there are more nonspeaking children than speaking children in these schools. this could indicate that because the lack of verbal speech is relatively easily identified by parents, they tend to send nonspeaking children to school at a relatively young age. international prevalence data by burd et al. (1988) suggests that changes in oral communication in special schools are unlikely after the age of 7. as the highest percentage of nonspeaking children in this survey was older than 7 years, the data could suggest that the possibility of changing from nonspeaking to speaking is slight. another factor confirming this finding is that a lack of speech by school entry is predictive of a poor outcome for learning to speak (burd et al., 1988). emphasis on aac systems should therefore be a priority in special schools as such a high percentage of children within these schools are aac candidates. regarding the gender of the nonspeaking children, a binomial procedure (non-parametric test) was applied to the data, which had a value of > 226, implying that there are significantly more nonspeaking boys than girls. the ratio of 2:1 (male:female) which was found in the total group of children prevailed in the nonspeaking group were a male:female ratio of 2,1:1 was reported. in other similar surveys, the male:female ratio was not as significant as in this survey, e.g., 1,4:1 in the australian survey (bloomberg et al., 1990), 1,6:1 in the scottish survey (murphy, markova, moodie, scott & boa, 1993) and 1,6:1 in the german survey (coon et al., 1992). consequently it can be stated that in this study significantly more boys than girls were described as nonspeaking. 2. communication abilities regarding the communication abilities of nonspeaking children, a number of factors were significant predictors of non-speech. table 4 describes the results obtained where the variables related to communication were tested against non-speech by means of the loglinear model. the interaction between non-speech and receptive language is highly significant. in cases where the children displayed very poor or poor receptive language skills, they spoke significantly less than 15 words, and when they distable 4: communication abilities of the children variable nonspeaking or speaking z-values (z = _ _ 2,58) variable nonspeaking or speaking very poor poor some reasonable normal receptive language nonspeaking speaking * 7,177 -7,177 * 3,052 -3,052 -3,456 *3,456 -4,645 *4,645 -1,366 1,366 ability to speak sentences nonspeaking speaking *8,545 -8,545 0,135 -0,135 -3,047 *3,047 -3,738 *3,738 -0,706 0,706 understandability of communication attempts nonspeaking speaking *2,758 -2,758 1,885 -1,885 0,971 -0,971 -4,645 *4,645 -1,366 1,366 au variables that indicate a significant correlation with nonspeech on a 95% confidence level, are marked with an asterisk (*). 1 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43,1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 juan bornman and erna alant played some to reasonable function, they spoke significantly more than 15 words. the generally poor expressive ability is further highlighted by the poor ability to use sentences (i.e. putting together two words or more). results indicated that nonspeaking children (thus children who were speaking 15 words or less) had a very poor ability to use sentences (they were thus not verbally putting any words together). this would indicate that speech is so limited for school purposes that it precludes functional oral communication. children speaking more than 15 words, however, were rated as having some, or reasonable functioning in speaking sentences. regarding how understandable teachers rated the children's communication attempts, it was found that the understandability of the nonspeaking children's attempts were rated as very poor, whilst the understandability of the speaking children's communication attempts were rated as reasonable. teachers were asked to rate communication desire with the following question "does the child try to communicate with people?" from figure 3 it is clear that when looking at nonspeaking children's communication desire it became evident that 89% of them did have the desire to communicate. lack of communication desire can thus not be interpreted as a major factor contributing to the high prevalence of nonspeaking children. a significant interaction between means of communication and non-speech was found when a pearson chisquare test was performed on the data. results are shown in more detail in figure 4. aac systems (including typing, writing and/or communication boards) were used only 0,79% of the time in the nonspeaking population (this percentage was rounded off to 1%). in comparison with the other means of communication that was used at least 8% of the time, it is clear that aac systems were used significantly less (figure 4). the second lowest means of communication is that of understandable speech (8%). these two variables are significantly lower than the third lowest means of communication, touching and gestures which are both present in 17% of the cases. the nonspeaking children used vocalizations as their most frequent means of communication (20%) followed by facial expressions (19%) and head nodding to indicate yes/no (18%). when looking at the high percentage of nonspeaking children, and the fact that 89% of them indicated a desire to communicate, it is to be expected that the number of children who use some sort of aac system to compensate would also be high, but this was not the case. the use of any aac system, aided (e.g., typing, writing or communication boards) or unaided (e.g., gestural systems) occurred infrequently in nonspeaking children (0,79% of the time). none of the nonspeaking children used typing, writing or high technology systems. the german study (coon et al., 1992), reported that the majority of children used unaided aac systems, with facial expressions as the means most frequently used, followed by gestures, touching and pointing respectively. this correlates with the findings of the present survey which reported that 17-19% of children used facial expressions, head-nodding to indicate yes and no, natural gestures and touching (see figure 4). these results are further supported by results from the washington state survey, which also reported that nonspeaking children used gestures/ emotional reactions as the predominant communication system (matas et al., 1985). this could be due to the fact that in the past nonspeaking children did not receive aac system intervention, and had to rely on natural gestures to make their needs known. the lack of skills training in the past thus resulted in children mostly using natural ways of communicating, as these natural ways were demonstrated in their environments (e.g., most people use facial expressions, head-nodding and natural gestures). furthermore, these unaided systems are, of course, always readily available. however, as people begin to realize the importance of communication intervention for nonspeaking children with the consequent development of cognitive abilities by giving nonspeaking children entrance into abstract thought, the use of more formal aided and unaided systems will increase, as it gives these children access to more extensive communication systems. in the past, in south africa, much emphasis was placed on the use of speech as the only way of communicating. for a great number of people the ability to communicate is equal to the ability to speak. nonspeaking children could not express themselves verbally and were therefore less frequently understood by teachers and their communication ability was also described as significantly poorer than that of speaking children (see table 4). the most frequently used means of communication were vocalizations (unintelligible speech) for the nonspeaking group, followed by facial expressions (19%) and head nodding (18%) (see figure 4). these findings correlate with those of the german study which also reported that speech was rated as the major means of communication (coon etal., 1992). this figure 3 : communication desire figure 4 : communication means the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) peaking children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater p r e t o r i a area: implications for speech-language therapists 59 vht be a reflection of the attitudes regarding aac sysf m s w h i c h prevail where information regarding these 6 s t e m s is limited and where they are mostly unknown. c o n t r a r y to research which has proved that the use of aac stems enhances communication and facilitates speech d e v e l o p m e n t , uninformed people hesitate to use aac systems as they fear that this might inhibit or prevent speech d e v e l o p m e n t as the aac systems might reduce motivation for speech communication (silverman, 1995). thus, in c o u n t r i e s where service providers do not have knowledge and skills regarding aac systems and aac does not have a strong basis for service delivery the tendency to aim at speech development was observed. this tendency was not noted in any of the other countries in which aac had been in use for some time (murphy et al., 1993 and matas et al., 1985). the general abilities of the total group of children are displayed in figure 5 and figure 6. in both of these figures a rating scale of 1 5 was used. one is indicative of very poor to almost no functioning, 2 of poor functioning, 3 of some functioning, 4 of reasonable functioning and 5 of normal functioning for chronological age (bornman, 1995). 3. general abilities in different skill areas percentages displayed on figure 5 and figure 6 were calculated to be proportionally correct for each variable. each variable discussed thus represents the proportional percentage. results from figure 5 show that children whose cognitive ability was rated as very poor spoke significantly less than 15 words. on the other hand, children who were rated as having reasonable cognitive functioning spoke significantly more than 15 words. results from figure 5 also show that for both gross and fine motor ability there was a significant interaction between these motor abilities and non-speech. results indicated that with very poor and poor gross motor functioning children spoke significantly less than 15 words, and with reasonable to normal functioning children spoke significantly more than 15 words. children who were rated as having very poor or poor fine motor abilities spoke significantly less than 15 words whilst children with reasonable fine motor abilities spoke significantly more than 15 words. j the loglinear model also showed a meaningful interac120 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 2 3 4 5 g r o s s m o t o r 1 2 3 4 f i n e m o t o r 1 2 3 4 c o g n i t i v e μ n o n s p e a k i n g ( n = 1 5 8 ) ί β s p e a k i n g ( n = 2 5 4 ) legend: 1 = very poor; 2 = poor; 3 = s o m e ; 4 = r e a s o n a b l e ; 5 = normal for chronological a g e . tion between communication (with both adults and peers) and social ability with non-speech, as displayed in figure 6. when communication ability with adults was rated as very poor to poor, children spoke significantly less than 15 words, and when communication ability was rated'as reasonable to normal, children spoke significantly more than 15 words. exactly the same tendency was found in the case of communication with peers, but the significance was even stronger in the latter case. a highly significant interaction between non-speech and social ability was also found. children who spoke less than 15 words were regarded as having very poor to poor social ability, and children speaking more than 15 words were rated as having reasonable to normal social ability. it is clear from the results obtained in the present survey that although the children's abilities varied in the different skill areas (motor, cognitive, communication and social), certain tendencies prevailed. the data indicated that when speaking and nonspeaking children's abilities were compared, nonspeaking children were rated as having significantly poorer abilities than the speaking children. this finding suggests that the abilities of the nonspeaking children might have been underestimated due to the fact that teachers do not understand these children well. as confirmation of the above, the poor communication skills of the nonspeaking children inevitably lead to poor social skills. it was clear from the results that the nonspeaking children's social abilities were mostly rated as "poor" and "very poor" whilst the majority of speaking children's abilities were rated as "good". this could be due to the fact that communication attempts from the nonspeaking children are ignored, overlooked or misinterpreted, which results in poor understanding of their intentions, which leads to increased isolation. figure 7 differentiates between the type of therapy (speech, occupational or physio-) received by the nonspeaking children as opposed to the speaking children as well as to whether the therapy was conducted on an individual or group basis. 4. therapy apart from physiotherapy, nonspeaking children received less speech and occupational therapy than their speaking counterparts. furthermore, children received group speech and occupational therapy rather than indi1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 1 0 0 2 3 4 5 1 2 3 4 c o m m u n i c a t i o n w i t h a d u l t s w i t h p e e r s μ n o n s p e a k i n g { n = 1 5 8 ) w / δ s p e a k i n g ( n = 2 5 4 ) legend: 1 = very poor; 2 = poor; 3 = s o m e ; 4 = r e a s o n a b l e ; 5 = normal for chronological a g e . figure 5 : cognitive and motor abilities figure 6 : communication and social abilities die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43,1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 juan bornman and erna alant vidual therapy. however, regarding physiotherapy, the majority of children received individual therapy. the majority of children receiving physiotherapy was nonspeaking. the therapy provided to the nonspeaking children varied from less than half an hour per week to more than five hours per week. the majority of children receiving therapy also received group rather than individual therapy. it could be due to the fact that most schools in the survey had a limited number of therapists, resulting in high caseloads. in order to address these caseloads and to serve as many children as possible, therapists mostly provided group therapy. the effectiveness of the therapy was not measured in this study, but the high number of nonspeaking children possibly suggests that the current therapy is not as effective and meaningful as it could be. it is furthermore important to note that aac is a relatively new field in south africa, therefore a strong basis for the use of aac systems within special education has not yet been laid. until recently speech therapists received no training in the use of aac strategies and thus, although 29% of the children received some kind of speech therapy, the nature of this therapy is unclear (see figure 7). i apart from requiring special training in working with children with special needs, service providers require familiarity and experience with the range of aac systems, aids, symbols and strategies available in order to equip them with knowledge and skills related to aac j (blackstone, 1990). as aac is a relatively new field of expertise in south africa, not many therapists and/or teachers (the primary aac service providers) receive any training in aac techniques during their undergraduate training (alant and emmett, 1995). in scotland, however, where aac has been in use for some time, the scottish survey also reported that the training of slp's should be addressed, as they do not receive knowledge and skill training related to aac (murphy et al., 1993). knowledge and skill alone are not sufficient, there is also a strong need for the development of interdisciplinary aac service delivery. this can be developed by exposing slp students to work with professionals from other disciplines in order to help them develop a more holistic approach to aac (huer, 1991). in the present study it was found that nonspeaking children received considerably less therapy than speaking children (see figure 7). as previously mentioned this might be due to the fact that service providers do not have the knowledge and skills necessary for working with this specific population. it might also be due to the attitudes 160 1 4 0 " 120 1 0 0 80 6 0 4 0 20 0 1 3 9 6 5 s p e e c h o c c u p a t i o n a l p h y s i o i n d i v i d u a l μ n o n s p e a k i n g ( n = 1 5 8 ) s p e e c h o c c u p a t i o n a l p h y s i o g r o u p ^ ^ s p e a k i n g ( n = 2 5 4 ) ' figure 7 : type of therapy received of service providers that nonspeaking children have a poor prognosis of developing in the various skill areas and thug the focus of service delivery is placed on the speaking children. these attitudes are interlinked with low expectations of nonspeaking children, as service providers often do not expect any progress from them (shrewsbury, lass and joseph, 1985). finally, another factor negatively affecting the services provided to nonspeaking children is the lack of a supportive infrastructure. therapists often work in isolated contexts and resources are not readily available. some schools included in the survey did not have therapists, and service provision is the responsibility of the teachers. as teachers do not have the skills and training to implement aac strategies without a supportive infrastructure, the implementation of these strategies become a low priority. conclusions nonspeaking children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater pretoria area constitute a large population with heterogeneous characteristics and abilities, who need intensive, specialized therapy and aac services. however, for a number of reasons, the nonspeaking population is underserved. this is due to the limited number of therapists working in special education (see table 1) as well as limited specialized knowledge and skills regarding aac strategies and low expectations from nonspeaking children due to attitudinal problems (shrewsbury et al., 1985). the results of this survey indicate a strong need for additional in-service and undergraduate training for professionals who serve the nonspeaking population. further research is also necessary to help improve the quality of service delivery to the nonspeaking population. acknowledgement the financial assistance of the centre for science development (hsrc, south africa) towards this research is hereby acknowledged. opinions expressed and conclusions arrived at are those of the author and are not necessarily to be attributed to the centre for science development. references aiello, s.c. (1980). non-oral communication survey: a onecountry needs assessment and demographic study. unpublished educational study. california: plavan school. alant, e. and emmett, t. (1995). breaking the silence : communication and education for children with severe handicaps. pretoria : human sciences research council. blackstone, s.w. (ed.): (1990). how you can use demographics to provide better service delivery in aac. acn news, 3(4), pp 1-7. bloomberg, k. and johnson, h. (1990). a statewide demographic survey of people with severe communication impairments. aac augmentative and alternative communication, 6(1), pp 50-60. bornman, j. (1995). nonspeaking children in schools for children with mental handicaps in the pretoria area: a survey of communication and related problems. .m.communication pathology thesis. pretoria: university of pretoria. (unpublished). burd, l., hammes, k., bornhoeft, d.m and fisher, w. (1988). a north dakota prevalence study of nonverbal school-age children. language speech and hearing services in schools, 19(4), pp 371-383. cantwell, d.p. and baker, l. (1985). speech and language: the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ν nspeaking children in schools for children with severe mental disabilities in the greater p r e t o r i a area: implications for speech-language therapists 6 1 d e v e l o p m e n t and disorders. in calculator, s.n. and bedrosian, j (eds). communication assessment and intervention for adults with mental retardation. boston : little b r o w n and company. _ r kremer, g. and hildebrand, m. (1992). a demographic analysis of people with speech impairments in west berlin, germany. unpublished presentation at isaac biennial conference, philadelphia. g r o e n e w a l d , j.p. (1986). social research: design and analysis. stellenbosch: university publishers and booksellers. huer m.b. (1991). university students using augmentative and alternative communication in the usa: a demographic study. aac augmentative and alternative communication, 7(4), pp 231-239. joseph, d. (1986). the morning. communication outlook, 8(2), 8. kopenhaver, d.a. and yoder, d.e. (1992). literacy learning of children with severe speech and physical impairments in school settings. seminars in speech and language, 13(2), pp 143-153. lloyd, l., fuller, d. and arvidson, h. (1988). glossary. in: lloyd, l., fuller, d. and arvidson, h. (eds). augmentative and alternative communication. new york: allyn bacon. matas, j.a., mathy-laikko, p., beukelman, d.r. and legresley, k. (1985). identifying the nonspeaking population· a demographic study. aac augmentative and alternative communication, 1(1), pp 17-31. murphy, j., markova, i., moodie, e., scott, j. and boa, s. (1993). augmentative and alternative communication systems used by people with cerebral palsy in scotland: a demographic survey. final report to the scottish council for spastics. stirling: university of stirling. shrewsbury, r.g., lass, h.j. and joseph, l.s. (1985). a survey of special educators' awareness of, experiences with, and attitudes towards nonverbal communication aids in the schools. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 16(4), pp 293-298. silverman, f.h. (1995). communication for the speechless. needham heights : allyn and bacon. skuy, m., westaway, m., makula, n. and perold, c. (1988). development of a screening instrument for the identification of pupils with impairments. south african journal of education. 8(1), pp 45-49. unicef. (1993). children and women in south-africa: a situation analysis. johannesburg: unicef and ncrc. yach, d. (ed). (1991). challenging health in south africa: ibwards new perspectives in research. california: the henry j. kaiser family foundation. appendix a : description of questionnaire ii description of childrens' abilities rating scale (if any) identification data: gender, age. * explanatory notes on categories can be found in bornman (1995). areas of functioning cognitive motor sensory (visual & auditory) communication, social 1 = almost no function 2 = poor function 3 = some function 4 = reasonable function 5 = normal function communication skills receptive language expressive language communication mode j 1 1 receptive language abilities were scored on a 5 point likert scale that ranges from a child who understands very limited instructions to a child who understands everything. expressive language was scored in terms of the number of words the child was able to produce intelligibly. if the child was unable to produce more that 15 words intelligibly, he/she was labelled as nonspeaking (burd et al., 1988). communication means included verbal, gestures, facial expressions, sign language, pointing to pictures, objects and symbols, writing and or typing, yes/no indication and vocalization. a question on the frequency with which the teacher understood the communication of the student was also included. reading skills ! j 1 = no interest in books 2 = interest in books/likes handling them 3 = sight vocabulary more than 30 words 4 = reads easy sentences, but not age appropriate 5 = adequate reading for chronological age writing skills 1 = no interest in writing or scribbles 2 = makes letters and letter like shapes 3 = makes letters and tries to write words 4 = writes most things, but not age appropriate 5 = adequate writing skills for chronological age number concept 1 = cannot count, or counts without number concept 2 = number concept up to 5 3 = knows concept "more hess" up to 5 4 = able to do simple mathematics, but not age appropriate 5 = adequate mathematical skills for chronological age therapy received: speech therapy occupational therapy ( physiotherapy a filter question was used (yes, no) as not all children received therapy. in applicable cases teachers had to specify the type of therapy received, and whether it was groupor individually-based. e suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43,1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) occupational therapists •user friendly speech therapists •windows / win 95 physiotherapists •full financial summary psychologists yy. y r,6i-m0n •no training necessary nurses (7 / ()s2-4i6-~i24 • \ccounts accepted /γ* /ft) γ / /wr by all mcdical aids wikrdapark south 0057 are you tired of your accounting system looking for one? is the printing of accounts a do you know what is going on with your is the input of account details a can you see all patient particulars with the press of a can you get a financial summary of any given metlvba^e/s the answer user friendly a windows / w r in 95 ^ j ν / \ no training necessary \ /accounts accepted by all medical aids requirements 386, 486, pentium 8 mb ram windows or win95 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so called 'lateral isl': some observations from palatography and spectrography a. traill m. litt. (edin.) african studies institute, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary this paper supplements certain well-known aiticulatory aspects of sigmatisms (lisps) with observations derived from spectrography. the five cases studied show widely differing articulations with a relatively invariant acoustic o u t p u t . a phonological perspective of the problem is argued to be of relevance t o therapy. opsomming hierdie artikel vul sekere bekende artikulatoriese aspekte van sigmatisme (stoot-met-dietong) aan met waamemings uit die spektrografie. die vyf gevalle wat bestudeer is, t o o n grootliks verskillende artikulasietipes met 'n akoestiese uitvoer wat relatief onveranderlik is. daar word aangevoer dat 'n fonologiese beskouing van die probleem relevant is vir terapie. in this paper an attempt is made to throw some new light on the familiar problem of lisping (or sigmatism). this will be approached in two complementary directions, firstly phonetically and secondly phonologjcally. certain familiar but possibly not sufficiently widely known facts about the articulatory aspect of a class of sigmatisms will be reviewed and data from a number of case studies will be interpreted in terms of the traditional classification. the acoustic correlates of these articulations will be presented and their phonological implications assessed. implications of the findings for therapy will be hinted at. the articulatory aspect of sigmatisms for the purposes of this paper, certain oral (i.e. non-nasal) sigmatisms will be discussed. these are conventionally classified according to the two phonetic parameters of place of articulation and direction of release of the airstream, leading to classifications varying in detail, completeness and usefulness. for various examples of different classifications the reader is referred to van riper and irwin,1 3 powers, 1 0 bloomer1 and luchsinger and arnold.s the latter present the most general and explicit classification and provide airflow and palatographs backing to their observations. the oral sigmatisms most frequently described in the literature can be summarised in table i. in addition it is often noted that the lips may be involved in some degree in sigmatism, though precise details of the type of labial articulation are not specified. regarding the gaps in table i, the writer ventures to suggest that die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 a. traill articulatory position release inter-dental 'anterior' (dental & alveolar) palatal central i.d. lisp addental lisp palatal/retracted lisp lateral i.d. lat. lisp r/l ) lateral bilateral ) lisp pseudo-lateral antero-lateral lisp table i. a classification of sigmatisms. only that of the pseudo-lateral palatal reflects an articulatory limitation; the others are apparently 'accidental' gaps. in the remainder of this paper the inter-dental sigmatisms will not be discussed. luchsinger and arnold8 (pp. 564-568) define the remaining sigmatisms of table i as follows: addental: . . . the tongue-tip . . . (is pushed) . . . against the posterior surface of the front teeth . . . formation of a medial groove along the tongue-tip becomes impossible, and the air escapes broadly between the teeth in a fan-shaped manner over the anterior portion of the tongue. lateral: . . . the tongue is elevated on one side more than the other. . . so that air is forced into one cheek pouch where it produces a slurping lisp. antero-lateral (lateroflex, pseudo-lateral): . . . the tongue-tip deviates from the midline toward the right or left side and directs the airstream mostly toward the upper canine tooth of the same side. palatal: . . . occurs when the tongue lies too far backward. in that case the fricative noise originates between tongue-tip and hard palate . . . the palatal [s] therefore sounds like a german [ς] or even a [/]· these authors' injunction to use precise visual and experimental exploration for the exact differentiation of the different types of sigmatism is worth repeating since faulty or imprecise diagnoses are inimical to adequate therapy. as an example of the subtle differences that can be revealed by palatography, for example, consider the articulatory differences between two cases of antero-lateral lisps for [s] and [ / ] given by luchsinger and arnold,8 and reproduced as figure 1 opposite. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' 5 ['•] [f] figure 1. palatograms of fine detail distinguishing sigmatisms (luchsinger and arnold if acoustic data were available to supplement the articulatory information provided by the palatograms, a precise and objective classification of the sigmatisms could be given. i shall now examine palatographic data from five cases of sigmatisms from the speech and hearing clinic of the university of the witwatersrand. the point of the exercise will be twofold: firstly it will provide a test of the adequacy of the classification of sigmatisms above; secondly it will provide the articulatory facts with which acoustic data can be correlated below. all subjects are english speaking. the shape of the dental arch has been standardised for all cases. case 1 sigmatism affecting /s/, /z/ only. diagnosed as 'lateral s' figure 2. palatogram for case 1 [s]. the hatched area in figure 2 and subsequent palatograms represents the area of articulatory contact between tongue and palate. the anterior part of the closure is asymmetric, being slightly retracted on the left side. otherwise this woiild be the sort of wipe-off one would associate with an alveolar point of articulation. there is extensive and asymmetric condie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 a. traill tact from the alveolar ridge posteriorly. the passageway through which the airstream is channelled is a wide groove (or narrow slit) for the greater part of its length, fanning out posteriorly in the palato-velar region. the length of this wide groove is significant, stretching from the palatal to alveolar point of articulation. the groove is situated entirely to the left of the midline of the palate so that the airstream appears to be aimed against the left upper incisor. this articulation deviates from that of a normal [s] in a number of respects: the asymmetry in the anterior point of articulation and the related deviation from the mid-line of the palate of the wider groove; the length of the wide groove; the abnormally wide area of contact between tongue and palate. for comparison, a typical palatogram for a normal [s] is given in figure 3. the auditory effect of this sigmatism is something like the blade palatal fricative [?]. on this criterion it would be luchsinger and arnold's8palatal sigmatism. however, the palatographs data clearly shows that this is not the case; the anterior point of articulation is alveolar, not palatal. this sigmatism is therefore strictly not classifiable in the above classification. case 2 sigmatism affecting /s/ /z/, / / / / $ /, and / ? / / /st,/. diagnosed as 'lateral s\ figure 3. palatogram for normal [s]. figure 4. palatogram for case 2 [s] and [/]. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' 7 the articulatory details of this sigmatism are the mirror image of those above. the anterior point-of-articulation asymmetry is on the right side and the wide groove is displaced to the right of the midline. again, the anterior point of articulation is more alveolar than anything else, and the bulk of the airstream appears to be aimed at the (right) upper central incisor. the impressionistic effect is also that of the blade palatal [9] although, as with the first case, the point of articulation is not straightforwardly palatal. however, close visual examination as well as attempts to locate the points of exit for airflow reveal something of a fanning of the airstream which is not clear from the palatogram above. in other words air is aimed at the upper lateral incisors as well as the left upper incisor. the articulation involves the blade of the tongue. the sigmatism for [ / ] involves exactly these details but has in addition lip rounding. case 3 sigmatism affecting /s/ /z/ only. diagnosed as 'lateral s\ careful inspection of the palatogram in figure 5 suggests some lingual contortions. the articulation is in fact achieved by a considerable displacement of the lower jaw to the right, that is to the same side from which the sound is released. the anterior point of occlusion is from the alveolar ridge to the teeth on the left, with the tongue tip in contact with the posterior surface of both upper incisors. however, on the right side the anterior point of closure is alveolar. this extreme asymmetry is achieved by twisting the tip of the tongue to the right so that its left edge has a wide and long contact with both the lateral and front teeth. the right edge of the tongue is therefore brought into a shorter though not exactly narrow contact with the palate from the first primary molar to beyond the second. the point of release is onto the right canine and the first molar. the passageway for the airstream is a slit displaced to the right of the midline for the greater part of its length and lines up with the right upper incisor. however, at the alveolar ridge the passageway extends bilaterally. release centrally and to the left is blocked by the left anterior closure, but release to the right is'unhindered. the total effect of the articulation is to produce a passageway that looks like a capital t, with the stem lying to the right of the midline of the palate. the auditory effect of the articulation could only misleadingly be called that of the lateral fricative figure 5. palatogram for case 3 [s]. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 a. traill [ £ ] ; the point of release of this sigmatism is anterior to that for conventional [£-] and this no doubt contributes to the peculiar auditory effect. the deviations from normal [s] are as follows: the left-right asymmetry in the anterior closure; the abnormally wide area of contact between tongue and palate, leading to a slit to the right of the midline of the palate, which is then diverted laterally at the alveolar ridge. in other words, if the right-left asymmetry was removed, and central release effected, the result would be a slit fricative released onto the right upper central incisor. this sigmatism is clearly a right antero-lateral (or lateroflex or pseudolateral) one in terms of the classification above. case 4 sigmatism affecting /s/. diagnosed as 'lateral s'. the left to right asymmetry of the anterior point of articulation is again achieved by twisting the tip of the tongue to the right so that complete closure is achieved between the left edge of the tongue along the lateral teeth, and then along the alveolar ridge across to the upper right (primary) lateral incisor. as with case 3, the right anterior point of articulation is slightly retracted from the alveolar ridge. the point of release is approximately the upper right canine. the passageway through which the airstream is directed is a very short, wide groove adjacent to the canine. the auditory effect is, once again distinct from [£-]. this sigmatism is also a clear case of a right anterolateral one but with significant articulatory differences from that of case 3. the deviations from [s] are: left-right asymmetry of the anterior points of articulation and a slightly widened groove. notice, however, that if-the asymmetry could be corrected by maintaining the point of articulation and groove but re-positioning the tongue-tip symmetrically round the midline of the palate, no further adjustments would be necessary for an almost perfect reproduction of the articulation for normal [s] (see figure 3 above). this is because there is a relatively small amount of contact between the edge of the tongue and the palate posterior to the alveolar articulation. in this respect the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' 9 case 4 differs from all those above (as well as case 5 below) by having a passageway that narrows anteriorly in an abrupt fashion. the other cases, it will be recalled, had either long wide grooves or a long slit resulting from broad and long contact between the edges of the tongue and palate. case 5 sigmatism affecting /s/ /z/. diagnosed originally as 'lateral s\ but the diagnosis was uncertain at the time of examination. figure 7. palatograms for case 5 [s]. the anterior point of articulation is just behind the upper incisors for (a) and roughly alveolar for (b). the striking point about the articulations is the width of the passageway through which the airstream flows. in (a) it reaches the primary upper lateral incisors while in (b) it stretches between the canines. in (a) there is naturally more contact between tongue and palate than for (b). however, this sigmatism involves a double articulation, the oral one above and a labial one shown in figure 8 below. figure 8. the labial component of sigmatism of case 5. when the lower lip is withdrawn during a prolonged articulation as in figure 9 next page, the extremely anterior placement of-the tongue-tip relative to the upper incisors can be seen, as well as the slit articulation shown in the palatogram in figure 7 above. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 a. traill figure 9. removal of the labial articulation during the production of a prolonged [s] by case 5. the auditory impression given by this articulation is that of the so-called whistling double fricative [d], when the labial articulation is disturbed as in figure 9 above the auditory impression is that of the slit fricative [r], although in one case the point of articulation is anterior to the conventional alveolar one for [r] and there is clearly more of the lateral part of the tongue involved. the articulation is deviant firstly in the slit rather than groove articulation and the fact that the slit, although symmetrical along the midline of the palate is unvaryingly wide along its entire length. the anterior point of articulation is too advanced for an alveolar consonant in one case. finally, the labial comt ponent is gratuitous in the normal articulation. this articulation does not have a strict counterpart in the classification above. while its anterior articulation is possibly addental in the one case it is alveolar in the other and there is no fan-shaped passage for release of the airstream. the intrusion of a labial articulation in sigmatisms is noted in traditional treatments, although in this case the contribution of this additional articulation is specifically a bilabial fricative, a possibility not explicitly discussed. with the exception of case 5, all the above sigmatisms involve a significant distortion of the articulation of the midline of the tongue relative to the midline of the palate, a fact documented clearly by mcglore and profitt9 whose conclusions were derived from strain gauge measurements. only case 5 has no such deviation, but, significantly only case 5 has a labial articulation as well as the oral one. mcglore and profitt9 point out that the normal tendency to bilateral asymmetry in tongue pressure is exaggerated and distorted in the cases of lispers. they attribute this to . . . poor co-ordination of tongue movements. another articulatory characteristic to emerge from the, above palatogramsis the general failure of the subjects to achieve the correct amount of contact between the sides of the tongue and the palate and teeth, a problem which has as its immediate effect an ill-proportioned passageway through which the airstream is channelled. in some cases it results in a wide groove incorrectly placed, in others a slit and in others an anterolateral deviation. only in case 4 is there something approaching a correctly dimensioned narrowing but this has been rotated to the right and is therefore wrongly positioned. a question of considerable interest for both traditional articulatory phonetic classification and the accurate description of the sigmatisms above, arises the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' 11 when one attempts to describe their place of articulation. for consonants, place of articulation is generally assigned to the point or points of maximum constriction in the supraglottal vocal tract (velic closures do not count). the question is to what extent this criterion is applicable in cases 1—5. case 5 is straightforward and represents a double articulation bilabial and dental in one case and bilabial and alveolar in the other, these labels identifying the two points of maximum constriction. (that these divergent articulations represent a single phonological entity is, of course, not so straightforward.) case 4 is problematic: we have referred to the direction of release of the airstream as anterolateral. the point of maximum constriction is clearly opposite the right lateral incisor and thus in the vicinity of the alveolar ridge. the fact that there is contact between the edge of the tongue and the front teeth, that is, the presence of dental articulation, seems to be only a mechanical consequence of the distortion of the midline of the tongue towards the right. in a peculiar sense, therefore, case 4 has a tip alveolar articulation. notice therefore, how inappropriate the conventional label 'lateral' is for this articulation. whereas lateral segments like [ i i λ] are classified as dental, alveolar and palatal respectively, with the airstream passing out over the edge of the tongue, in case 4, the airstream passes along the centre of the tongue. the latter observation applies to case 3 as well, though, as noted, this case differs in other respects from case 4. the point-of-articulation problem presented by case 3 is that the slit along which the airstream passes does not have any obvious point of maximum constriction so far as its lingual articulation is concerned. however, the slit is abruptly constricted by the right lateral incisor, again in the vicinity of the alveolar ridge. this poses a dilemma: because of the tongue's deviation to the right the articulation is dental but alveolar, a situation which traditional phonetics is not able to resolve adequately. cases 2 and 1 both present the problem that although they both involve an anterior point of articulation that is alveolar, the point of maximum constriction is not confined to that point. in both cases the wide groove stretches from a palatal to an (asymmetric) alveolar point of articulation without altering its dimension significantly. thus the grooves are not only uniform, they are extremely long. to give an approximate but clearer idea of the length of these grooves compare the section profiles next page for [s] and the sigmatisms under discussion. the profile for [s] is from heinz and stevens,6 the others are constructions based on the palatograms in figures 2 and 3. from a traditional articulatory point of view, the point of articulation for the two sigmatisms is indeterminate; it is thoroughly abnormal. notice that the traditional secondary articulation of palatalisation is an inappropriate label for cases 1 and 2 because of the degree and extent of the constriction. a more revealing characterisation of this sort of articulation can be provided by examining its acoustic effect, a point to which we turn below. for the reader who is impatient to learn what the relevance is of this excursion into fine articulatory phonetic detail, it should be pointed out that it is the necessary groundwork for later conclusions of a phonological nature. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 a. traill [s] (from x-ray) (approximate) case 1 case 2 (approximate) figure 10. section profiles for normal [s] and two sigmatisms.. when the phonological status of the sigmatisms is assessed it will have to be done against a well-founded set of articulatory and/or acoustic features. the importance of establishing this set in the precisest terms possible will become most apparent when the fundamental question of degree of deviation is raised. the acoustic aspect of sigmatisms this section will be confined to examining only certain aspects of the acoustic effect of the above articulations with a view mainly to supplement the articulatory descriptions so that later phonological conclusions will be adequately grounded. the production and acoustic effect of turbulent airflow in the vocal tract has been closely studied for example by heinz and stevens,6 stevens 1 2 and fant.4 a number of factors are considered in the analysis of articulations that result in turbulence. these are the length of the cavities posterior and anterior to the constriction, the length of the constriction, the cross-sectional area of the constriction and, in stevens' terms, the degree of acoustic coupling between the two cavities. the interaction of these factors and their acoustic output can be calculated in a precise manner from an idealised model of a constricted vocal tract, yielding results which fit well with data derived from the study of actual vocal tracts (stevens1 2). the applicability of these findings to the cases under discussion is confounded by the lack of data on cavity sizes for each case, as well as the unknown effects of the abnormal articulatory configurations, in particular the irregular constrictions and cavities/it is thus not possible even to estimate a valid correcting factor in order to accommodate stevens' predictions. therefore the few interpretations made below that draw on these predictions should be judged against the reservations expressed. it should be noted, however, that the main conclusion i wish to draw from the acoustic data is not affected. figures 11 16 are broad band spectrograms of english words recorded by cases 1 —5, illustrating the various sigmatisms. table ii presents an analysis of these and other spectrograms in terms of the upper and lower limits of the thesouth african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' figure 13: case 3 : sand figure 14: case 3 : song r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 a. traill fricative noise in the spectrum as well as the major spectral peak(s) in this range. the latter were derived from a section taken at a point immediately prior to the release of the constriction and the onset of the vocalic segment. figures in brackets refer to a section taken at a point about 8 m. sees earlier in the segment. underlined figures represent the greatest peak. the average given under the column 'range' is the averaged lowest frequency of the range. for comparison, averaged figures for 2 normal children of comparable age to the subjects are included in table ii. the striking feature about all these sigmatisms is that their spectral energy is concentrated relatively high and there is most frequently no energy at lower frequencies. this characteristic places the sigmatisms firmly in the same category as normal [s] and [/], the very segments of which they are deviant renderings. if one considers the range of articulations that underlie this acoustic unity, this is a noteworthy feature. it shows that a number of articulatory positions and adjustments are compatible with the single acoustic feature of energy at relatively high frequencies. this property is lacking in the other english fricatives [f ν θ the property has been labelled stridency (e.g. halle5) or sibilance (laaefoged7). stevens 12proposes that the basis for the distinction between the two classes of fricative is the presence in the strident set of a . . . coincidence between a pair of resonances . . .; that for particular points of constriction . . . corresponding to two coincident resonances, a maximum is obtained in sound output and the resonant peak is relatively insensitive to small perturbations in the positions of the constriction. there exist, as it were, certain discrete or quantal constriction positions (places of articulation) for sound production. the articulatory and acoustic details of the sigmatisms may be interpreted in the light of these quotations as showing that none of them involves a transgression of the optimisation point for the production of a particular acoustic result. this has been achieved articulatorily by directing the turbulent airflow at roughly right-angles onto the upper teeth or incisors which therefore provide a sharp obstacle and greater noisiness. it is known that quite considerable differences exist between languages and within a language for acceptable amounts of tongue-palate contact in the production of normal [s] and [ / ] (dieth3), but it is clear that none of the above sigmatisms can be considered to fall within the limits of this normal variation because of the excessive amount of contact and, for some cases, grossly asymmetric contact. it is likely that peculiar features of the abnormal constrictions themselves contribute to the particular effect, but this point will not be pursued in any detail here. a few observations on each case follow. / / case 1 there is a clear concentration of energy above ~ 3,6 khz and none below it. the spectrum is however flat, without any! prominent major peak. generally the greater energy is located around 6,0 khz but it falls away only very gradually around this point. the suggestion of an upward bending where f2 is expected, in the spectrogram for song, is consistent with a palatal point of articulation, but the location in the section of a peak of energy as high as 6 khz suggests a more anterior point of articulation. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' 15 sigmatism range (khz) to 8,1 major peaks (s) (khz) case 1 'palatal s' 4,0 5,5 to 7,0 (6,0) case 1 'palatal s' 3,6 4,5 to 7,0 (5,25) 4,0 5,25 to 6,5 (6,0) 4,1 4,75 to 6,25 (6£) (av. 3,19) (av. 5,8) case 2 'palatal s' 1,75 (5,30 and 6,25 and 8,25) case 2 'palatal s' (4,25) (5,30 and (\5 and 8,20) 2,3 4,8 and 6,4 (3,25) (5,0 and 6,25 to 7,0 and 8,25 3,50 6,25 to 8,0 3,55 4j5 and 6,5 and 8,25 (av. 3,1) (av. 5,2; 6,36; 8,19) 'palatal / ' 2,5 5,0 and 6,5 and 8,0 'palatal / ' 2,25 4,25 and 6,4 and 8,0 ,60 4,0 to 5,0 and 6,3 and 7,5 (3,50) (4,5 and 6,25 and 7,75) (av. 2,87) (av. 4,56 6 , 3 6 ; 7,81) case 3 'antero-lateral s' 3,60 4,9 and 6,25 and 7,5 (2,00) (2,6 and 4,25 and 6,0 to 7,5) 2,40 3,4 and 5,1 and 6,6 (1,75) (3,0 and 5,25 and 6,5 to 7,0) (av. 2,44) (av. 3,5; 5,21; 7,00) case 4 'antero-lateral s' 3,6 4,1 and 6,0 to 7,25 3,8 4 j and 5,1 and 7,4 (3,7) (4,25 and 5,5 and 7,1) 3,8 4,4 and 6 £ and 7,0 to 7,6 (av. 3,7) (av.4,21 5 , 8 0 ; 7 , 2 6 ) case 5 (a) 'double s' 1,0 5,0 in words 0,75 5,0 0,60 5,1 (2,4) 5j0 (av. 1,19) (av. 5,0) (b) 'double s' 3,0 4j8 in isolation 2,75 4,4 and 6,4 and 7,7 (av. 2,87) (av. 4,6; 6,4; 7,7) normal 1 averages [s] 4,0 5,27 7,22 [/] 2,5 3,26 6,15 7,81 normal.2 averages [s] 4,0 6,25 8,50 [/] 2,5 3,60 7,50 table ii. an analysis of the range and concentration of spectral noise during the production of various sigmatisms. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 a. traill the latter observation is consistent with the palatographic evidence, the former with the possibility mooted above that a point more posterior within the constriction is contributing to the production. whatever the case may be, the flat envelope is not normal for sibilance on the one hand, but its high location is, on the other. this case's productions of [ / ] are thoroughly normal with a lower peak corresponding to the coincidence of f3 and f4, and a cut off point at 2khz. case 2 although the palatogram for this case is the mirror image of that for case 1, there are a number of differences. firstly, both [s] and [f] are affected; secondly, case 2 has much more clearly defined peaks of energy in the sigmatisms; thirdly, case 2's articulation often shifts just prior to release of the consonant, with the effect of spreading the spectrum to lower frequencies of very weak intensity. this latter point is clearly seen by examining the figures in table ii for the sections taken at a point before the shift is evident acoustically. the acoustic difference between the two sigmatisms involves firstly a generally higher intensity for the sigmatism of [s] as well as a slightly wider envelope for the sigmatism of [ / ] (2,87 khz vs. 3,1 khz for the [s] sigmatism) and a lower peak of energy situated at 4 , 5 6 khz for [ / ] (ch. 5,20 khz for fs]). these differences can be attributed solely to the effect of the lip-rounding found only in the [ / ] sigmatism. the addition of this anterior cavity serves to lower the resonances slightly. however the differences achieved in this way do not approximate normal differences between [ / ] and [s] and in fact, the auditory impression is mostly of identity. if one mechanically manipulates the lips so as to prevent the formation of a labial resonating cavity, no difference exists. so far as point of articulation is concerned, this is indeterminate for traditional categories because f2 bending and f4 and f5 amplification point to the possibility of a complex source for the turbulence. this evidence suggests that use of the label 'palatal' distorts both the articulatory and acoustic reality. this accords with the palatographic evidence. both sigmatisms qualify as sibilants with a frequency range beginning very high up. it is of interest to note that the data for case 2 were collected on separate occasions about eight months apart and considerable differences in intensity are found between intensity levels for the [ / ] sigmatism, with the later sample showing less intensity than the first one. although this will not be pursued here, this sort of comparison is highly suggestive for longitudinal assessment of treatment. , / case 3 compared with the spectrum for normal sibilants and for all the sigmatisms except that of case 5, case 3's spectrum is wide, with energy as low as 1,75 khz. there are clear peaks of energy however and generally two of these are located at ~5,2lkhz and ~7,00khz. the location of a peak of energy at ~3,30khz, however, is abnormal for [s] and is undoubtedly attributable to the peculiarities of the airway and its anterior deviation. this lower peak would be expected with normal [/], but auditorily, its effect is balanced by the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' 17 the high-frequency peaks consistent with [s]. despite the abnormality noted, this sigmatism clearly meets all the acoustic requirements for classification as a sibilant. the balance between [s]like and [/]like acoustic features relies on the location of the energy peaks in the spectrum for the fricative. in so far as the data are clear, stevens' criteria would suggest a balance has been achieved in fig 13 but that [/]like properties predominate in fig 14. unfortunately no data is available on this case's normal [ / ] . . case 4 this sigmatism involves peaks of low intensity at ~4,21khz, 5,80khz and 7,26 khz. like the previous case, this suggests a combination of [s]like and [/]like characteristics and depending on the intensity of the lower peak v. the higher peaks, one or other will predominate. for instance in fig. 15 where the section shows no clear resolution of the higher frequencies [s]like amplifications are missing in the transition to the vowel, but the peak found at ~4,0 khz is amplified into f3 and f4 suggesting [/•]like properties; other spectrograms, however, display [s]-like properties. this may be due to inconsistent articulations, but it could also reflect peculiar effects of the orientation and shape of the constriction. once again, traditional articulatory labels fail to provide an accurate categorisation. case 5 the presence of low-frequency, noise is due to the turbulence generated at the hps. in addition, this constriction evidently serves to amplify frequencies with great intensity and consistency around 5,0 khz. removing the labial constriction removes certain low-intensity noise probably generated by the lips. emerging from 5 khz in the transition to the vowel are f4 and f5 which in stevens' terms points to post dental constriction. this is of interest because it suggests a narrowing there that is not visible either from the palatograms or the picture of the position of the tongue-tip relative to the upper incisors (figure 9). one may speculate that the deep and wide groove evident in that picture extends only a short way posteriorly, possibly to the alveolar ridge, where a shallower constriction lies. although this is a double fricative with labial and dentalalveolar points of articulation and a wide groove, it is evident from the acoustic data that traditional articulatory categories do not really help to elucidate the nature of the sigmatism. it is noteworthy that this sigmatism qualifies as a sibilant like all the cases surveyed. the strongly focused, high location of an exceptionally high peak of energy is the mark of a sibilant according to the definition used in this paper, and although there is low-frequency-lowintensity noise it is effectively neutralised from a linguistic point of view by the other feature. the acoustic properties of [ / ] for this case are normal with clear spectral peaks at 2,5 khz and 5,0 khz. a certain amount of low-intensity noise at all the lower frequencies of the spectrum points strongly to the lips being involved in this articulation, although this was not noticed during the recording session. the phonological aspect of the sigmatisms in this section an attempt will be made to assess the functional significance of die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafykings, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 a. traill the sigmatisms for the phonological system which underlies them. the point to be emphasised is that all the articulatory and acoustic phonetic data examined are of little interest in their own right. their true value, strictly speaking, can only be determined by examining how they are structured phonologically. to put it differently, as linguistic sounds, these sigmatisms must be seen as expressing some phonological contrast, and they lose all significance if treated outside such a context. the only way to arrive at an accurate assessment of their severity in the sense of their disruptive effect on the phonological system, is to view them in their proper linguistic context. this in turn will lead to implications for a controlled therapy routine. the english fricative series is as follows: f θ s / v £ ζ 3 the system of oppositions that structure the series involves the presence or absence of voice and articulatory position. there is, however, the need for an additional feature in order to provide a natural explanation for the distribution of the plural and 3rd person singular allomorphs /—3z/ and /—ζ—s/: safe s path s kiss 3 ζ fish 3z latch 3z cave ζ tithe ζ fizz 3z garage 3z badge 3z /—> /"ν for this rule, english treats the class of final consonants / s ζ / 3 t / d 3 / a s a natural one; they all take the /—3z/ allomorph. in order to explain this, one searches for a single feature that they all share in common which is lacking in the complementary class of / f ν θ β /. the feature is of course 'sibilance' (or stridency) and it has its phonetic basis in articulatory and acoustic details that have been mentioned though it is actually defined in acoustic terms only (ladefoged7). to express this in current terms, each fricative represents a bundle of distinctive features, the relevant ones of which can be displayed as follows: f v θ £ s ζ / 3 voice + + + + sibilance • + + + anterior + + + + + + the boxed-in fricatives are those involved in the sigmatisms discussed above and we may now ask how these so-called articulatory defects disrupt the above system. with the exception of case 2, the answer is not at all. cases 1 , 3 , 4 , 5 , show that despite widely different articulatory details, the sigmatisms are sibilants; the voice and position features are not affected. case 2 differs in the latter respect; the bundles <+ sibilant ± anterior>have been collapsed to a single sibilant using the tongue blade. that is, there is no positional contrast the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' 19 involving the feature<+ sibilant>for case 2. instead, labialisation is the surrogate for the pair of features γanterior"! l + labial j in other words, case 2 has restructured the physical instantiation of the sibilant fricatives. the question is whether this has been successful and, as noted above, the answer is that it has not. clearly, the difference between the normal expression of /s/ and / / / cannot be achieved through lip rounding alone; a positional shift is necessary in order to create a larger anterior cavity for the particular resonances associated with normal [j]. case 2 therefore disrupts the system of fricatives at a systemic level and must to that extent be regarded as the most serious of the cases presented. this disruption is reflected below: f v θ * c j voice + + + sibilant + + anterior + + + + coronal + + (the symbols c and j cover normal [ s / ] and [ ζ respectively) it was noted above that the feature 'sibilance' is customarily given a clearly acoustic definition and i would suggest that in a sense the correctness of this emphasis is borne out by the fact that each case has managed to preserve the acoustic feature of sibilance under quite different articulatory configurations. this points strongly to the reality of the feature as the target in the child's acquisition of english fricative contrasts, and is further confirmation of the entrenched or resistant nature of the feature in various disorders (cairns and williams2). case 2 can be seen therefore to have a truly linguistic problem as against what are probably best described as the cosmetic problems of the other cases. these cosmetic problems are attributable to deviations from the norm of fine acoustic detail which arise from various kinds of articulatory distortion. put differently it is clear that cases 1 , 3 , 4 and 5 completely satisfy the requirements of english fricatives at the phonological and phonetic level and to that extent they must be regarded as normal. it is true that the abnormalities which lead these cases to be diagnosed in the first place as having a speech defect are indeed expressed in phonetic substance, but it must be understood that these phonetic deviations are not sufficient to disrupt the linguistic system and, in a sense, are therefore most superficial. their linguistic triviality may in fact partly explain why sigmatisms are so resistant to therapy: at a certain level, there is no reason to change! however, the fact is that such cases nevertheless require therapy and one may ask whether the observations made in this paper would favour any particular programme. it seems they do. they suggest that the articulatory distortions themselves should be the explicit and direct focus for therapy, rather than die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommuniksieafivykings, vol. 24 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 a. traill auditory feedback. the basis for. this emphasis is that the cosmetic distortions derive directly from the articulatory facts of abnormal constrictions and abnormal orientations. at an auditory level the output of these articulatory configurations is fully acceptable linguistically and therefore likely to be highly resistant to change. it would be reasonable instead to aim directly at articulatory adjustments which would not only preserve the linguistically necessary acoustic output, but at the same time remove the fine but offensive acoustic deviations. by way of a practical suggestion, one may suggest that normal [s] could be 'grafted on' to the release of the voiceless alveolar stop since, firstly, the sides of the tongue would be normally orientated, secondly, release would be central, and thirdly the friction associated with an aspirated release could be transformed into a linguistically and cosmetically acceptable [s]. initial [s] could be introduced in this way by juxtaposing words with initial [s] after words with final [th]. this sort of approach could establish the correct kinaesthetic feedback, arid indeed one would expect just this in terms of the motor theory of speech perception. case 2, obviously requires an approach which takes into account the linguistic and any cosmetic deviations, but it would seem that this could also be done through an articulatory programme. references 1. bloomer, η. h. (1971): speech defects associated with dental malocclusions and related abnormalities. in handbook of speech pathology and audiology, travis l. e., (ed.). new york: appleton-century crofts. 2. cairns, h. s. and williams, f. (1972): an analysis of the substitution errors of a group of standard english-speaking children. j. speech hear. res., 15,811-820. 3. dieth, e. (1950): vademekum der phonetik. bern: a. frank. 4. fant, g. (1970): acoustic theory of speech production. the hague: mouton. 5. halle, m. (1959): the sound pattern of russian. gravenhage: mouton. 6. heinz, j. m. and stevens, κ. n. (1961): on the properties of voiceless fricative consonants. j. acoust. soc. amer. 33,589-596. 7. ladefoged, p. (1971): preliminaries to linguistic phonetics. university of chicago press. 8. luchsinger, r., and arnold, g. e. (1965): voice, speech, language: clinical communicology: its physiology and pathology. belmont: wadsworth. 9. mcglore.r.e. and profitt,w.r. (1973): patterns of tongue contact in normal and lisping speakers. j. speech hear. res. 16,456-473. 10. powers, μ. h. (1971): functional disorders of articulation symptomatology and etiology .handbook of speech pathology and audiology, travis, l. e., (ed.). new york: appleton-century crofts. 11. stevens, κ. n. (1971): airflow and turbulence noise for fricative and stop consonants: static considerations!. j. acoust. soc. amer. 50, 11801192. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) on so-called 'lateral [s]' 21 12. stevens, κ. n. (n.d.): acoustic correlates of place of articulation for stop and fricative consonants. quarterly progress report no. 89, research laboratory of electronics, massachusetts institute of technology. 199-205. 13. van riper, c. and irwin, j. v. (1958): voice and articulation. pitman medical publishing co., london. hearing aids — widest range of world-renowned makes audiometers — wide range of screening — portable — diagnostic — clinical — automatic and research models silent cabins — singleand double-wall cabins in all sizes. ventilation — one-way glass — and many other features auditory trainers — transposing apparatus — articulation amplifiers — group training equipment — speech reproducer — delayed speech reproducer — peep show accessories — visible speech apparatus — sound-level meters — plus our comprehensive service throughout the country also manufacturers of audiometry and speech training equipment h e a d o f f i c e : 2 1 2 h a r l e y c h a m b e r s , c o r n e r j e p p e a n d k r u i s s t r e e t s , j o h a n n e s b u r g . telephones: 3 7 2 6 4 3 / 4 / 5 p.o. b o x 2 2 6 9 , j o h a n n e s b u r g 2 0 0 0 telegraphic address: b o n a v o x die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings, vol. 24, 1977 s.a. (pty) ltd for r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a division of s.a. philips (pty) ltd. hearing aids portable audiometers [roup teaching systems philips head office 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street, p.o. box 3069, j o h a n n e s b u r g . @ ) 4 4 9 2 3 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 24, 1977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of a group of hearing impaired children alida m . u . laubscher, m . s . washington university introduction recently there has been a great surge of interest and activity in the area of developmental psycholinguistics. this discipline, exemplified by the work of brown and bellugi2, ervin (1964), mcneill11, menyuk (1963, 1964a, 1964b) and others, has largely arisen because of the theoretical expositions of chomsky (1957) who provided the formulations concerning the grammatical structures of the english language. this led researchers to apply these principles to the language acquisition of children. the psycholinguists hold that children have an innate capacity to acquire most syntactic rules in the short span of life from one and a half to three and a half years of age. it is a more accurate explanationof language acquisition than the theories of imitation. evidence that imitation alone does not account for the phenomenon of language acquisition, is the observation that a normal, hearing child at the age of three or four does not produce only sentences which he has heard, but generates his own novel set of sentences. what he acquires is the group of rules that governs the generation of the sentences. he learns a finite set of rules that enable him to produce an infinite set of sentences. this process of indirectly learning the syntactic rules has been called induction of the latent structure by brown and bellugi2. early grammatical rules the generative grammar model as formulated by chomsky has been applied in the literature to describe the language acquisition of the child. brown and fraser3, braine (1963) and miller and ervin (1964) have investigated the earliest two-word combinations of normally developing children and have identified the use of pivot words as the child's first grammatical construction. the two-word combinations' were called sentences. the word combinations were found not to be random, but to follow a pattern, and the words were categorized as belonging either to the open class or to the pivot class: membership in both pivot and open class is heterogenous from the point of view of being part of speech membership in the adult language. the words in the pivot class emerge later into grammatical classes similar to that of adult grammar. brown and bellugi2 have described the development of the nounphrase as a grammatical unit in children's grammar. building on brown journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of a g r o u p of hearing impaired children 19 and bellugi's data, mcneill (1965) proposed that the earliest grammatical constructions are noun phrases and predicate phrases and on occasion, full sentences. the early speech of children reflects the properties of the phrase structure of sentences. it does not reflect the operation of transformational rules; these seem to come into children's grammar at a later stage. transformational development a transformational model of structure was used by menyuk (1963, 1964a and 1964b) to describe children's grammar in the age range from two to seven years to indicate developmental trends. it was found that four-year-old children used a greater number of different transformations per child than three-year-olds. it seems that .a four-year-old is less fixated as to the number of different sentence types than a child one year younger. evidence of the four-year-olds' advanced linguistic competence was seen in their decreased use .of simple active-declarative (kernel) sentences as compared with the three-year-olds. children mature toward finely differentiated linguistic competence in stages of advancement. the first sentence rules seem to be those relating to phrase structure; these are learned by two to three years. the second stage in learning the rules governing the generation of sentences seems to be that of simple transformations. the final level of maturity would be the mastery of complex transformations. morphological development development of morphophonemic rules, governing pronunciation and relating the syntactic and phonological parts of the grammar, has been investigated; the findings have indicated certain developmental trends. berko (1966) found the children's usage of noun plurals, verb inflections and possessives indicated a clear developmental trend. the children's performance on verb inflections indicated best performance on the progressive. the past tense of the irregular verb form was never used correctly by the four and five-year-olds and only one child of the sixtyone five to seven-year-olds made use of this construction. because the division between regular and irregular verbs is an arbitrary one, rote learning is required in each case. cazden (1968) found that the irregular forms are often used correctly by the child before he starts to differentiate between the rules. the application of ons rule for all forms is often found, for example, "went" and "go-ed" or "walked" and "weared". this overgeneralization is followed eventually by the correct use of the regular and irregular verb inflections. cazden (1968), reporting on the development of three verb inflections, found that the present progressive appeared first in the utterances of all three of his subjects; complete mastery was reached between approximately twenty and forty months. the sequence of acquisition of the tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 alida μ . u . laubscher regular past and present indicative was less consistent, relative to age and rate of acquisition. the studies described above, which utilized a generative model of grammar seem to agree with the hypothesis that the child has incorporated both the generative rules of the grammar and a heuristic component that samples an input sentence, and by a series of successive approximations determines which rules are used to generate a sentence. also, there appears to be a clear developmental sequence of certain features. if this is an innate capacity that the child brings to the task of acquiring language, it must also exist in the hearing-impaired child. he should possess a source of hypotheses as rich as a hearing child's, since in both cases hypotheses result from the capacity for language acquisition rather from the language itself. the limitation placed on the hearingimpaired child, however, is the input of the sentence constructions. t language acquisition of hearing-impaired children the main trend of description of the language of deaf children in the past has been based on the rules of traditional grammar. the application of linguistic rules to describe the language of deaf children indicated that deaf children show many features in their language production that some have termed "deafisms": stereotypy, syntactical errors, limited vocabulary and wrong choice of words. fries's method of reclassifying parts of speech into four classes and function types, based on their privilege of occurrence in sample sentences, has been a useful procedure in analysing the linguistic performance of hearing-impaired children. studies utilizing this method have been completed by simmons (1962), goda (1964) and mcginitie (1964). studying a group of eight to fourteen-year-old deaf and hearing children, simmons indicated that the deaf exceeded the hearing in the use of class i words (nouns), class ii words (transitive and intransitive verbs) and determiners. the hearing children used more class iv words (adverbs, auxiliaries and conjunctions). the two groups used a similar number of class iii words (adjectives and prepositions). the deaf used prepositional groups of place and accompaniment. comparing the number of different words spoken by each group, the hearing group used a higher type-token ratio than the deaf. mcginitie (1964), using a sentence completion test, indicated that the deaf children show a nearly uniform retardation in supplying content and function words. these results; are not in complete agreement with the earlier studies by heider and heider and myklebust who indicated that deaf children omit abstract or function words which have no tangible referents. language of deaf ohildren contains many nouns and verbs which tend to be more concrete. while hearing impairment seems to create under-use of some word classes, it results in a tendency to over-use nouns and articles. hearing impairment interferes with the learning of function words more than journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of a g r o u p of hearing impaired children 21 with the learning of content words. among the content words, adverbs are found to be an exception. function words tend to be more redundant than lexical words. cohen (1967) found that deaf children construct messages with less redundancy than is generally found in the language of their hearing peers. these investigations indicate that 'hearing-impaired children are deficient in both specific verbal skills and in general language. when the deaf child's verbalizations are more than rote performances or memov rized strings of words, his utterances will reflect a system of formulational rules. these rules can be analysed and the developmental sequence of language can be illustrated. the studies mentioned above described specific structural features in the language of deaf children. a developmental sequence was not indicated. goal the goal of the present study was to determine whether there are developmental trends of certain language features and formulistic rules in the language of hearing-impaired children. language development of normal children indicates that certain language features are acquired before others and show improvement in correct usage as a function of age. from the language samples of this group of hearing-impaired children, indications of occurrence of certain features and language rules as well as improvement in correct usage was analysed. method the language samples of the ten hearing-impaired children in this study represent the age range from five to three years and nine to seven years. the mean hearing level of the subjects, in the better ear, was 76db i.s.o. the children were all enrolled in the school programme at the central institute for the deaf, st. louis. prior to entrance, they were participants in a parent-infant programme. language appropriate to the children's linguistic age levels was used in the school programme. language was taught through experience; stories which .were written following an actual group experience were used. the traditional approach of systematic teaching of structural forms was not followed. language was elicited from the children who were instructed to talk about a picture. this was tape-recorded. the procedure followed in analysing samples was directed toward counting the occurrence of certain language features at five age levels. the language samples of each child were analysed with reference to the following features: (a) the use of single word, two word and three word combinations. (b) kernel sentences. according to the generative grammar model, a kernel sentence is any single sentence which is generated without the application of optional transformations. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 alida μ . u . laubscher (c) verb tenses and verb inflections. the occurrence of the present tense, past tense (irregular verbs) present and past progressive aspects was counted with reference to which form occurred earliest in the language samples. a comparison of the frequency of occurrence was also made. (d) the transformations conjunction, conjunction deletion, the auxiliary "have", pronominalization, particle displacement, the comparative ' and passive voice were examined. the age at which the transformations occurred was determined. the usage of sentences, possibly indicating pre-transformational sentence structures, was analysed. results verb tenses and inflections. the occurrence of the present tense, the present progressive aspect, the past tense and the past progressive aspect indicated a clear developmental trend. the present tense occurred earliest in the children's language samples. the present progressive aspect and the past tense (irregular verbs) appeared second and third respectively. although there was not a significant difference in the time of occurrence of the present progressive aspect and the past tense (irregular verbs), there was a significant difference in the frequency of usage of the two forms. the present progressive aspect was used as the dominant form when both occurred in all the language samples. this was especially true for the age range of five to seven years. the past progressive aspect appeared in the language samples at the nine year level. this verb form never appeared before the others. the present tense was always used in all language samples when verbs were present. as age increased, the present tense was not the main verb form of discourse, and was used less frequently. other verb forms became the main verb forms of discourse. a developmental sequence of verb forms appeared to be present. table i summarizes the occurrence of verb tenses and verb inflections in the language of the subjects. all the language samples, except for two out of thirty-one, indicated the same pattern. the present tense preceded or occurred .with the present progressive aspect. the past tense occurred after the present progressive, and the past progressive was the last form to appear. this sequence is representative of the group as a whole. in the two samples in which the developmental sequence was not reflected, the present and past tenses occurred. subject iii used the past progressive aspect at the age of six years but not in the later samples. the appearance of the auxiliary "be" with the present progressive, and the inflection /-ing/ were included in the analysis because there appeared to be inconsistencies of' correct usage. omission of either the auxiliary "be" or /-ing/ occurred with the correct usage of the present progressive form. more omissions occurred at ages five, six and seven than at age levels eight and nine. the correct application of "be + ing" journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of a g r o u p of hearing impaired children 23 concurrently with an omission of either can be illustrated as follows: subject i i : "the boy is smiling and the girl smiling, . . . the dog is laugh . . . these results appear to be in agreement with the results of cazden (1968). the present progressive aspect and the present tense were acquired first in the language of his three normal hearing subjects. tihe auxiliary "be" plus the inflection /-ing/ occurred, with" omissions and correct usage at the same time and was not completely mastered at the age of forty-nine months. berko (1966) reported that at the age of four years the children used the present progressive correctly 90% of the t a b l e 1 : o c c u r r e n c e o f v e r b t e n s e s a n d v e r b i n f l e c t i o n years subjects 5 6 7 8 9 i ν pr τ pr pr a ii + pr τ p r t pr pr a pr τ pr pr a p a t > iii pr τ pr pr a p r t pr pr a p a t pa pr a pr τ pr pr a pr τ pr pr a iv ν ν ν v pr τ p r t vi ν p r t p r t pr pr a vii ν pr τ pr pr a p a t pr τ pr pr a p a t p r t pr pr a p a t pa pr a viii ν p r t p r t pr pr a p r t pr pr a p a t pa pr a i x ν p r t pr τ + p r τ p a t x pr τ pr pr a p a t p r t pr pr a p a t p r t pr pr a p a t pa pr a symbols: n : n o occurrence p r t : present tense pr pr a : present progressive aspect pa t : past tense (irregular verb) pa pr a : past progressive aspect tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24 alida μ . u . laubscher time. the past tense verb inflection of irregular and regular verbs was used correctly by a much lower percentage. the past tense of the irregular verb form was analysed because no evidence of the regular verb past tense inflection was found. it would require the allomorph /-ed/. in the training these hearing-impaired children received, they were exposed to all the verb tenses and inflections. verb tenses or verb inflections were not taught per se, but whatever form seemed appropriate was used. in general they had more exposure to the past tense of both regular and irregular verbs than the other three forms described in this study. the children used the following irregular verbs in the past tense: took, fell, were, had and got. these verbs were always inflected. when one looks at the acoustic dimension, the allomorph /-t/ that is required for the verbs used is a voiceless plosive and unstressed, and might be inaudible for the subjects'. the irregular verb inflection requires a change in the vowel and might be available auditorily. the printed form of both regular and irregular verbs was presented to the children in their language training. cazden (1968) reported that the irregular verb inflection is acquired by children first before the application of differential inflection rules . . . irregular forms may start out as separate lexical items with no past meaning for the child. in spite of the fact that these children had been exposed to both forms of verb inflections, only one form was present in their spontaneous language utterances as transcribed by several listeners. verb plus complement construction: the verb plus complement construction was analysed because the picture stimulus seemed to elicit the use of transitive and intransitive verbs in the subject's spontaneous utterances. the structural description of a sentence with a verb plus complement is as follows: sentence: s->np + vp n p = n o u n phrase v + c + (np) v + c = v e r b + complement c = particle prepositional phrase two sentences can be formulated. (a) noun phrase + verb + particle + noun phrase. (b) noun phrase + verb + prepositional phrase. in adult grammar, sentence (b) with transitive verbs /would not be used and a transformation, particle displacement would have to apply. the structural change of the sentence representing the transformation is: (c) noun phrase + verb + noun phrase + particle. the age and percentage of occurence of 'these three sentence constructions were determined. the percentage of children using the verb + complement construction suggests an increase of occurrence over years. at the eight and nine year level, it appeared in all the language samples and was used more frequently. the sentence structure, noun phrase + journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of a g r o u p of hearing impaired children 25 verb + prepositional phrase occurred in all the language samples of children using the construction. omission of the preposition occurred only during the early language samples of individual children. omission of prepositions appeared concurrently with the correct use of the preposition, followed soon by a stage of no omissions. menyuk (1964) reported that the child at the age of two years omits prepositions and uses them at two to six years of age, although not always where required. this stage is soon followed by correct application of prepositions. the prepositions used by the ten children in this study were all of place and accompaniment. menyuk (1964) found that the prepositional phrase of location is used before the prepositional phrase of time. feofanov (1958), reporting on the use of prepositions in children's speech (ages three to seven), found that the earliest and most often used prepositions were those having the greatest number of meanings. the three most common prepositions were "in", "on" and "with". the earliest usages expressed primarily spatial relations. the use of prepositions expressing relations of purpose, time relations and space relations used figuratively, appeared later on. the sentence structure, noun phrase + verb + particle + noun phrase which can be considered as a pre-transformational rule, was present in five language samples in the age range of seven to nine. the presence of this sentence structure may indicate that the children are using the same rule for particles with transitive and intransitive verbs. example: v + particle + np v + prepositional phrase take with the dishes look at the dishes put on the pan sit on the floor put up the pan stand up the introduction of the transformation particle displacement would indicate an advanced stage of development in that the differentiation of treatment of particles with transitive and intransitive verbs is made. the developmental sequence could be exemplified as follows: subject x stage i stage ii omission of particle v + particle + np girl and boy put the pan boy puts on the pan stage iii transformation particle displacement he puts the pan on . . . conjunction. the occurrence of conjunctions in the spontaneous language samples of this group of children indicates the usage of various rules. different levels of complexity of the use of conjunctions were found as age increased. the acquisition of conjunctions appears at an early age. the most frequently occurring conjunction was "and". conjunctions "because" and "so" were used only by two subjects at age levels six and nine. to tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 alida μ . u . laubscher describe the development of the rules as they increase in complexity, an analysis of the rules found in the language samples was undertaken. the rules are as follows: rule 1. ν + and + ν (n: noun). adj + and + adj. (adj: adjective). rule 2. np + vp + and + np + vp (np: noun phrase) (vp: verb phrase). conditions: 1 2 3 4 5 this is the conjunction of two kernel sentences in which all conditions are different, for example: the boy is laughing and the girl is crying 1—2—3—4—5 no condition in either kernel sentence can be deleted. rule 3. n p + v p + a n d + n p + v p conditions: 1 2 3 4 5 structurally rules 2 and 3 are the same, but in rule 3 certain conditions are identical. for example: the boy is laughing and the girl is laughing 1—2—3—4—5 conditions 2 and 5 are two verb phrases that are identical and condition 2 or 5 may therefore be deleted. conjunction of the two kernel sentences in rule 3 requires a transformation. the resulting transformation is conjunction deletion and is found in adult grammar. the use of rule 3 in children's grammar may be considered as a development stage before the acquisition of the conjunction deletion transformation. because the two rules are structurally the same, the child may apply this rule to all conjunctions of two kernel sentences before he acquires the differentiation between the two different and two identical conditions in sentences. rule 4. conjunction deletion no. 1. in this transformation condition 2 as described above is deleted. rule 5. conjunction deletion no. 2. rule 5 also requires the deletion of conditions from one of two combined kernel sentences. structural description: n p + v + n p + a n d + np + v + n p 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 the boy has socks and the boy has shoes structural change: transformation conjunction deletion no. 2. np + v p + n p + a n d + n p 1 2 3 4 7 the boy has shoes and socks condition 1 5 and 2 6 are identical and 5 and 6 must therefore be deleted to form the transformation conjunction deletion no. 2. in the language development of these children there was a sharp decrease in the occurence of rule 1, i.e. noun + and + noun. it ocjournal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of a g r o u p of hearing impaired children 27 curred in the early language samples of the children who used it. the rule was either replaced by the conjunction kernel sentence + and + kernel sentence or used concurrently with the other rules. the use of the conjunction with two different kernel sentences increased rapidly and appeared as the dominant rule in all language samples at age levels seven, eight and nine. rule 3 suggests a pre-transformation and would not appear in adult grammar. the use of rule 3 occurred at early age levels, six and seven years. this pre-transformational rule was soon either replaced by the transformation conjunction deletion or used concurrently with the transformation. transformation conjunction deletion no. 1 occurred in the age range five to nine years. it was used by only one child at age five. there was a steady increase in the number of children using it and in the frequency of occurrence. the occurrence of the transformation was preceded by rule 3 in the language samples of nine children. transformation conjunction deletion no. 2 or rule 5 occurred in the seven to nine year range. it was always used as a transformation. two children at ages six and seven used the conjunction "and" to string many nouns together in a sentence. example: the boy has some—and —sock and shoe and shoel—and pant and shoes and socks. this never occurred at the nine year level. menyuk (1963) reported on the development of transformations conjunction and conjunction deletion by normal hearing children ranging in age from 3-0 to 6-0 years. conjunction (e.g. rule 2 in this study) was used by 87% of the children at age three; conjunction deletion was used by 62% of the children at age three. at ages five and six, conjunction occurred in the language samples of all the children. conjunction deletion, however, was used less frequently but showed a steady increase in the number of children using it in the age range 3-8 to 6-0 years. the transformation conjunction is one of the few transformations used at the,three year level. development of syntax structures. the comparison of the major sentence structures used most frequently at the various age levels indicated a developmental sequence similar to that of normal children. the use of kernel sentences increases as the children become older. the use of the kernel sentence alone as the underlying rule of all sentences in a language sample decreases rapidly. at the age of eight, it was always used with other transformations by all children. at the age of seven, only two out of eight children used the kernel sentence to formulate all their sentences. there appears to be a rapid increase in the use of transformations concurrently with the kernel sentences. the transformation auxiliary "have" occurred at age level seven with correct inflection in the language samples of all the children. it was not used frequently however. the transformation pronoun occurred with high percentage and increased with age. very few errors occurred. for each individual child, there was an increase in the different kinds of pronouns used. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 alida μ . u. laubscher on the acquisition of the transformations auxiliary "have" and pronoun, menyuk (1963) and brannon (1967) reported a further development. the transformation auxiliary "have" did not occur in the language of two and three year olds and appeared at age four or later. the transformation pronoun was never used by three year olds and it seldom occurred at age four. the transformation auxiliary "have" is based upon the use of specific word endings and seems to be a more difficult morphological form to acquire. the presence of the transformation comparative and passive was also examined in the language samples of the ten subjects. the comparative was used by three children at ages five, six and seven. it occurred only once in each language sample. the passive was never used by any of the children. the use of the passive by normal hearing children as discussed by menyuk (1964) suggested an increase in the number of children using it over the age range of 2-10 to 6-0 years. at 2-10 years it was used by one out of eight children; at ages 3-6 to 3-9 it was used by 48% of the children and by age six by 85%. at age levels eight and nine, the children were using kernel sentences as well as a greater number of different transformations, as compared with the sentences formulated at the earlier age levels. of the six transformations examined, the pronoun and conjunction were the most frequently used and occurred at an earlier age. the introduction of more transformations in their language as this group became older indicates that they became less fixated as to the number of sentence types they were using in their spontaneous language utterances. discussion the results from the analysis of the language samples suggests a developmental sequence of language acquisition by the ten hearing impaired children. the data will only allow the observation of developmental trends, since the number of children and language samples were small. the language acquisition of the ten subjects appears to be similar to that of normal hearing children. the main difference is the chronological age at which the grammatical structures emerged. the usage of verb tenses and verb forms indicated similarity with normal hearing children as described by cazden (1968) and berko (1966). the,-appearance of the kernel sentence demonstrated the children's early syntactic development. the kernel sentence appeared after single word and two or three word combinations for some children. other children were already at the stage of using kernel sentences when their language was first recorded. the major portion of syntactic acquisition for normal children after kernel sentences is the growth of transformations. the ten subjects used •transformations more frequently as they became older. the transformation conjunction was one of the earliest acquired transformations. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of a g r o u p of hearing impaired children 29 similar results were reported by menyuk (1964). the appearance of conjunction deletion no. 1 was preceded by a pre-transformational construct. this appears to demonstrate a developmental stage in the formulation of rules by the children. the development of transformation particle displacement was preceded by the application of a generalized rule to combine verbs and particles. at a later age, a rule to. differentiate between transitive and intransitive verbs with particles appeared. in the school situation, the transformation particle displacement would have been incorporated. there appears to be indication of development from generalizing rules to correct application of the transformation. the transformation auxiliary "have" is acquired by normal children at age three or later. the hearing impaired children used it infrequently, and it appeared later than transformations conjunctions and pronoun. the prepositions that occurred in the hearing impaired children's spontaneous language were those of place and accompaniment. according to feofanov (1958), they are the first prepositions acquired by normal hearing children. the language acquisition of these children does reflect rote performances or memorized strings of words. these data lend support to the hypothesis that deaf children, as well as normal hearing children, have an innate capacitiy for language acquisition. lennenberg (1964) stated that: "deaf children could hardly differ in the capacity for doing this, from hearing children, provided they were given enough examples and are allowed to go through a natural order of grammatical development. this capacity to acquire language can be demonstrated by the rules and/or categorization of rules used to formulate sentences. the hearing impaired child is limited in his ability to test hypotheses against adult speech, and language exposure and instruction .is brought to the task of language acquisition. a training programme utilizing the theory of the innate capacity would necessitate the provision of many language constructs so that the deaf child would be given the opportunity of inducing the structural principles of the language in a natural way. the capacity to acquire language appears to be transitory (mcneill 1966). this suggests that concentrated language exposure and teaching should begin early, i.e. before the age of four, which is the age at which some investigators have said the child is at his peak of language acquisition. summary spoken samples of language from a group of ten hearing impaired children were analysed with respect to development of occurrence of verb tenses and verb inflections, the development of the kernel sentence and four transformations: conjunction, particle displacement, auxiliary "have" and pronoun. the language samples represented an age range of five—three to nine—seven years. the mean hearing level of the ten subjects, in the better ear was 76db i.s.o. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17,nr.: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 alida μ . u. laubscher from the results obtained, general conclusions seem to emerge. firstly, the earliest acquired verb tense is the present, followed by, or appearing concurrently with, the present progressive. secondly, the kernel sentence is correctly used at an early age by some children. as age increases, so does the percentage of children using the kernel sentence. from approximately seven years of age, more transformations are used. the transformations conjunction and particle displacement indicated a clear developmental trend towards greater complexity. the conjunction and pronoun transformations were used by a greater percentage of children. the transformation auxiliary "have" appeared much later and less frequently. · these developmental trends show strong similarities to the trends observed in normal hearing children by other investigators. there is, however, a time lag in that the hearing impaired children develop these features at a later age and over a longer period of time. opsomming spontane spraakvoorbeelde van 'n groep van tien gehoorgestremde kinders is ontleed met verwysing na die ontwikkeling van die voorkoms van werkwoordtye en verbuigings, die ontwikkeling van die kernsin en vier transformasies. die taalvoorbeelde verteenwoordig 'n ouderdoms omvang van vyf—drie tot nege—sewe jaar. die gemiddelde gehoorpeil van die tien proefpersone, in die beter oor, was 76db i.s.o. sekere ontwikkelingsneigings is gevind in die taal van die kinders wat ooreenkomste getoon het soos gevind deur ander ondersoekers by normale kinders. daar is egter 'n tydsverskil, in die dat die gehoorgestremde kinders die taalelemente op 'n later ouderdom en oor 'n langer periode van tyd ontwikkel het. bibliography 1. berko, j., t h e child's learning of english morphology. psycholinguistics ed s. saporta, 1966. 2. brown, r., bellugi, u., three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax harvard educ. rev., 1964, 34, 133-151. 3. brown, r., frazer, c., t h e acquisition of syntax. mon. soc. research in child development, 1964, 29, 43-78. 4. cazden, c. b„ t h e acquisition of noun and verb inflections. child development, 1968, 39, 433-448. / 5. chomsky, n., aspects of the theory of syntax. m.i.t. press, 1965. 6. feofanov, m. p., on the use of prepositions in child speech. genesis of language, m.i.t. press, 1966, 366-367. 7. fries, c. c., the structure of english: an introduction to the construction of english sentences, new york, 1952. 8. goda, s., spoken syntax of normal, deaf and retarded adolescents. journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 1964, 3, 401-405. 9. lennenberg, ε. h., biological foundations of language. wiley, new york," 1967. 10. macginitie, w., ability of deaf children to use different word classes. j.s.h.r., 1964, 7, 141-150. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of a g r o u p of hearing impaired children 31 11. mcneill, d., t h e capacity of language acquisition. volta review, 1966, 39, 17-32. 12. menyuk, p., alteration of rules in children's grammar. journal of verbal learning and verbal behavior, 1964, 3, 480-488. 13. , syntactic rules used by children from preschool through first grade. j child development, 1964, 533-546. 14. , comparison of grammar of children with functionally deviant and normal speech. j.s.h.r., 1964, 7, 109-121. 15. miller, w., ervin, s., t h e development of child grammar in child language. m o n . soc. res. child development, 1964, 29. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 85 spraakklankdistorsie by neuromotoriese spraakafwykings: 'n vergelyking tussen serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie isna erasmus anita van der merwe emily groenewald departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming spraakklankdistorsie word algemeen in die literatuur beskryf as 'n kenmerk van neuromotoriese spraakafwykings soos serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie. die doel van die studie is 'n vergelykende ondersoek na die temporale en ruimtelike aspekte van spraakklankdistorsie by twee persone met verworwe serebellere disartrie en een persoon met verworwe verbale apraksie. die spraakparameters stemaanvangstyd van die [d], afsluitingsduur van die [d], konsonantduur van die [s] en [i], formante van die [i]en die omvang van akoestiese energie van die [s], is spektrografies ondersoek in 'n aantal uitinge met verskillende klankstrukture. die resultate dui daarop dat ruimtelike en temporale distorsie van artikulatoriese bewegings voorkom by die proefpersone met serebellere disartrie sowel as by die proefpersoon met verbale apraksie. daar is egter verskille opgemerk in die aard en graad van die spraakklankdistorsie by die onderskeie afwykings. die teoretiese implikasies van hierdie verskille word bespreek met verwysing na 'n model van normale spraakproduksie. abstract speech sound distortion is considered to be a salient feature of neuromotor speech disorders such as cerebellar dysarthria and apraxia of speech. the aim of this study was to compare the temporal and spatial aspects of speech of two persons with acquired cerebellar dysarthria and of one person with acquired apraxia of speech. voice onset time of[d], duration of articulatory closure of[d], duration of [s] and [i], formants of [i] and the range of acoustic energy of is] were analysed spectrographically in a number of utterances with various sound structures. the results indicated that spatial and temporal distortion of articulatory movements occurred in all three subjects. however, differences in the nature and degree of speech sound distortion in the two different disorders were observed. the theoretical implications of these differences are discussed with reference to a model of normal speech production. spraakklankdistorsie word algemeen in die literatuur beskryf as 'n kenmerk van neuromotoriese spraakafwykings soos serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie (brown, darley & aronson,. 1970; kent, netsell & abbs, 1979; itoh & sasanuma, 1984; hardcastle 1987; odell, mcneil, rosenbek & hunter, 1990; odell, mcneil, rosenbek & hunter, 1991). serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie is egter die resultaat van die aantasting van verskillende neurale dele, wat betrokke is by verskillende stadiums of fases van die spraakproduksieproses. verbale apraksie word tradisioneel gesien as 'n afwyking in spraakprogrammering terwyl disartrie getipeer word as 'n afwyking in die uitvoering van spraakbewegings. daar is egter aanduidings in die literatuur dat serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie sekere ooreenstemmings toon in die aard van waargenome spraakklankdistorsies soos byvoorbeeld verlenging van woordsegmente, lettergreepspraak en temporale diskoordinasie van spraakstrukt'ure wat by albei afwykings voorkom (kent etal., 1979; kent & rosenbek, 1982). die rede hiervoor is onbekend. vorige akoestiese studies aangaande verbale apraksie en serebellere disartrie het slegs enkele aspekte soos vokaalformante en segmentele duur ondersoek en die vraag ontstaan presies watter temporale en ruimtelike aspekte van distorsie by die twee afwykings ooreenkom en verskil. 'n omvattende, objektiewe akoestiese analise van verskillende fasette van spraakklankproduksie sal distorsie by hierdie afwykings verder toelig. die presiese aard van spraakklankdistorsie kan aanduidings gee van die aard van die twee tipes neuromotoriese afwykings en ook lig werp op die bydrae van die kortikaal-motoriese dele en die serebellum tot die beheer van spraakbewegings. navorsing oor die spesifieke spraakkenmerke van serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie word hoofsaaklik gekenmerk deur twee eksperimentele ondersoekmetodes. hierdie metodes behels subjektiewe metodes soos bree fonetiese perseptuele transkripsies en objektiewe metodes soos akoestiese analises, elektropalatografiese studies en fibroskopiese studies. vroee subjektiewe ondersoeke die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 86 isna erasmus, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald aangaande serebellere disartrie is hoofsaaklik gerig op simptoombeskrywings. op grond van subjektiewe perseptuele studies word die kenmerkende simptome van serebellere disartrie in drie verbandhoudende hoofsimptoomgroepe verdeel, nl. simptome gekenmerk deur artikulatoriese onakkuraatheid bv. "onpresiese konsonante" en vokaaldistorsies, simptome gekenmerk deur prosodiese oormaat bv. oormatige en gelyke klem, verlengde foneme en stadige spraakspoed en simptome gekenmerk deur fonatories-prosodiese onvermoe bv. monotone spraak, monoluidheid en heesheid (darley, aronson & brown, 1969b; brown et al.,1970). grunwell en huskins (1979) beklemtoon dat hierdie simptome van serebellere disartrie deurlopend teenwoordig is en kumulatief van aard is. hulle beskryf die kumulatiewe aantasting van spraakverstaanbaarheid as 'n tekort aan intonasie en normale ritme met sogenaamde "scanning speech", verlengde konsonante, periodieke foutiewe stemgewing en nasaliteit as resultaat. hierdie perseptuele simptoombeskrywings slaag egter nie altyd daarin om tussen verskillende tipes disartrie te onderskei nie (zyski & weisiger, 1987) en die aard van die afwyking kom ook nie duidelik na vore nie. enkele ondersoeke met objektiewe analisemetodes soos akoestiese en kineradiografiese studies wat die aard van spraaksimptome by serebellere disartrie beter toelig, is uitgevoer. opsommend beskou, is die volgende waarnemings deur kent en netsell (1975) en kent et al. (1979) gemaak : verlengde artikulatoriese beweging van die tong, geringe vokaaldistorsie weens swak anterior-posterior aanpassing van tongposisie, konstante onakkurate artikulasie van dorsale afsluitingsklanke, verlengde segmentduur, gelyke en oormatige klem, afwykende fundamentele frekwensiewaardes en variasie in fundamentele frekwensiewaardes. vanuit hierdie resultate blyk dit dat spraakklankdistorsie 'n kernsimptoom is van serebellere disartrie. studies oor die spraakkenmerke van persone met verbale apraksie is kontroversieel van aard, deels as gevolg van die kwessie van die ontledingsmetode. aanvanklike studies aangaande verbale apraksie het hoofsaaklik gebruik gemaak van subjektiewe, perseptuele analiseprosedures soos distinktiewe eienskapsanalises en gemerktheidsanalises (klich, ireland & weidner, 1979). op grond van hierdie resultate is die kernsimptoom van apraksie dan beskryf as vervangings. nuwe navorsing deur middel van meer objektiewe studiemetodes soos akoestiese ontledings, getroue fonetiese transkripsies, en elektropalatografiese waarnemings dui egter daarop dat alhoewel ware klankvervangings ook deel vorm van die simptoomkompleks van verbale apraksie, baie van die "vervangings" wat in vroee studies gei'dentifiseer is, in werklikheid spraakklankdistorsies was (itoh & sasanuma, 1984; kent & rosenbek, 1983; hardcastle, morgan barry & clark, 1985; van der merwe, uys, loots & grimbeek, 1988; odell et al., 1990; odell et al., 1991). tans word dit algemeen aanvaar dat spraakklankdistorsie 'n kernsimptoom van verbale apraksie is. akoestiese studies van verbale apraksie en studies wat van direkte waarnemingsmetodes van die artikulators gebruik maak, verskaf direkte en meer presiese inligting aangaande afwykende motoriek. uit hierdie studies kan 'n goeie aanduiding verkry word van spraakklankdistorsies by verbale apraksie en die motoriese prosesse daarby betrokke. kent en rosenbek (1983) gee 'n simptoombeskrywing van 'n verbaal-apraktiese spreker op grond van foute gei'dentifiseer tydens akoestiese analises, nl. stadige spraakspoed met verlenging van klankoorgange, vermindering in intensiteitsvariasies oor sillabes heen, stadige en onakkurate beweging na ruimtelike teikens vir konsonanten vokaalproduksie, probleme met tydsberekening en koordinasie van stemgewing met die beweging van ander artikulators, periodieke foute met segmentseleksie of opeenvolging eri inisieringsprobleme. ander akoestiese ondersoeke van verbale apraksie bevestig die resultate van kent en rosenbek (1983) en vind byvoorbeeld ook afwykings in interartikulator-sinchronisasie en verlengde segmentele duur (sands, freeman & harris, 1978; hardcastle, 1987; itoh, sasanuma, tatsumi, murakami, fukusaki & suzuki, 1982; collins, rosenbek & wertz, 1983; kent & mcneil, 1987; van der merwe, uys, loots, grimbeek & jansen, 1989). uit die voorafgaande literatuuroorsig is dit duidelik dat spraakklankdistorsie as kernsimptoom by sowel serebellere disartrie as by verbale apraksie gei'dentifiseer is, maar dat die aard van die spraakklankdistorsies nog nie genoegsaam uitgelig is nie. vergelykende studies tussen verbale apraksie en serebellere disartrie kan meer lig werp op die motoriese aard van moontlike ooreenkomste en verskille in die spraakklankdistorsies. daar bestaan egter weinig sulke studies. in 'n resente perseptuele vergelykende ondersoek tussen serebellere disartrie, verbale apraksie en konduksie-afasie deur middel van objektiewe, getroue fonetiese transkripsies is kortliks gevind dat die persone met verbale apraksie en serebellere disartrie as 'n groep meer ooreenkomste toon met mekaar as met konduksie-afasie (odell et al., 1991). die verbale apraksie groep het egter minder distorsies vertoon as die serebellere disartrie groep, nl. 64% teenoor 81%, asook meer sillabeklemfoute en verlengings van intersegment klankoorgange. geen aanduiding word egter verskaf van die bewegingsaspekte betrokke by die waargenome foute nie. kent en rosenbek (1982) vind in 'n vergelykende akoestiese ondersoek dat sprekers met verbale apraksie sowel as sprekers met serebellere disartrie prosodiese afwykings soos artikulatoriese verlengings en sillabesegregrasie toon en verduidelik dit as die resultaatl van kompensasie vir die neuromotoriese aantasting. gillmer en van der merwe (1983) vind dat sowel sprekers 'met serebellere disartrie as sprekers met verbale apraksie afwykende stemaanvangstydwaardes (sat) vertoon en dat sat-waardes meer gevarieer het vir die sprekers met serebellere disartrie. weens 'n tekort aan objektiewe vergelykende studies en die feit dat bestaande studies slegs enkele spraakparameters ondersoek het, is daar tans steeds 'n tekort aan omvattende inligting betreffende die aard en omvang van spraakklankdistorsie by die onderskeie afwykings asook verklarings vir moontlike ooreenkomste. spraaksimptome soos spraakklankdistorsie wat na spesifieke neurale aantasting voorkom, kan gedeeltelik verklaar word aan die hand van kennis oor/die funksie van die betrokke aangetaste neurale deel. verbale apraksie en serebellere disartrie verteenwoordig letsels in verskillende dele en op verskillende vlakke van die brein. serebellere disartrie word met aantasting van die serebellum in verband gebring (kent & netsell, 1975; kent et al., 1979, grunwell & huskins, 1979). ten spyte van uitgebreide navorsing oor die funksies van die serebellum the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spraakklankdistorsie by neuromotoriese spraakafwykings 87 is daar egter steeds weinig bekend oor die rol daarvan in spraakmotoriek (gentil, 1990). posturale en neuromotoriese simptome geassosieer met serebellere letsels, nl. asinergie, disdiadokokinese van spraakbewegings, intensietremor gedurende beweging en hipotonus van spiere lei tot die ruimtelike en temporale distorsie van spraakbewegings tydens die uitvoerstadium van die spraakproduksieproses (darley, aronson & brown, 1969a). daar is egter aanduidings in die literatuur dat die serebellum ook betrokke is by hoevlak motoriese programmering van liggaamsbeweging deur middel van 'n oop serebro-serebellere kringbaansisteem (allen & tsukahara, 1974; brooks, 1986). vanuit so 'n teorie kan daar geredeneer word dat alhoewel die serebellum en die motoriese korteks nie dieselfde funksies in die motoriese beheer van spraak vervul nie, aantasting van albei areas moontlik kan lei tot probleme met die voorbereiding van 'n "motoriese program" vir spraakproduksie (kent & rosenbek, 1982). tradisioneel word die spraakproduksieproses beskryf as bestaande uit hoofsaaklik drie vlakke, nl. linguistiese enkodering, artikulatoriese programmering en laastens die uitvoer van bewegings (itoh & sasanuma, 1984). in die lig van die neurofisiologiese onderskeid wat gemaak word tussen motoriese beplanning en programmering, verskaf hierdie tradisionele model nie genoegsame verklarings en interpretasiemoontlikhede vir navorsing aangaande neuromotoriese spraakafwykings nie. van der merwe (1994) postuleer dat daar vier hoofvlakke van spraakproduksie bestaan, nl. linguisties-simboliese beplanning, motoriese beplanning, motoriese programmering en uitvoer van beweging. van der merwe (1994) maak dus, in teenstelling met die huidige tendens in literatuur van neuromotoriese spraakafwykings om "beplanning" en "programmering" as dieselfde begrip te hanteer, 'n duidelike onderskeid tussen hierdie terme. sy postuleer verder dat dit wel moontlik is dat die serebellum sekere progammeringsfunksies vervul in spraak soos die programmering van ruimtelik-temporale bewegingspesifikasies (byvoorbeeld spoed en tonus) voordat wcrklike bewegings plaasviiid, maar dat hierdie programmeringsfunksies verskil van' die beplanningsfunksies van die assosiasie-areas tydenjs spraakproduksie. brooks (1986) se uiteensetting van die motoriese hierargie bevestig so 'n teorie. op grond van die model van van der merwe (1994) is dit dus moontlik dat spraakklankdistorsie by serebellere disartrie die) resultaat kan wees van die aantasting van beide die uitvoeren programmeringsvlakke van die spraakproduksieproses. alhoewel daar nog nie uitsluitsel bestaan oor die funksies van kortikale en subkortikale motoriese dele geassosieer met verbale apraksie nie, postuleer van der merwe (1994) dat kortikale dele soos die area van broca, die kortikaal-motoriese assosiasie-areas en ook die posteriorparietale areas betrokke is by die motoriese beplanning van spraakproduksie. verbale apraksie reflekteer dan moontlik probleme met die beplanningsfase van spraakproduksie (van der merwe, 1994). in die geval van subkortikale skade, van veral sekere dele van die basale ganglia wat ook tot apraktiese simptome lei (kertesz, 1984), is dit egter moontlik dat daar ook afwykings in die programmering van spraakbewegings is. volgens die model van van der merwe (1994) kan spraakklankdistorsies by verbale apraksie dus die resultaat wees van probleme met die beplanning van ruimtelik-temporale spesifikasies van struktuurbewegings binne die grense van ekwivalensie asook met die programmering van bewegings. op grond van die model van van der merwe (1994) bestaan daar dus teoreties 'n moontlikheid dat sekere ruimtelike en temporale aspekte van die kernsimptoom spraakklankdistorsie soos wat dit voorkom by serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie, moontlik kan oorvleuel op die vlak van motoriese programmering. 'n tekort aan uitgebreide, objektiewe, vergelykende navorsing van die spraakkenmerke van persone met serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie, beperk egter die begrip van afwykende spraakproduksieprosesse betrokke by hierdie twee neuromotoriese spraakafwykings. 'n uitgebreide akoestiese ondersoek na beide die ruimtelike en temporale aspekte van spraakklankdistorsie by serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie, binne 'n omvattende teoretiese model, kan dus meer lig werp op die aard van hierdie neuromotoriese spraakafwykings. metode doelstellings die doel van die studie is die akoestiese ontleding van sekere spraakparameters in die spraak van persone met serebellere disartrie en verbale apraksie om so vergelykende data in te samel aangaande die voorkoms en aard van akoesties identifiseerbare spraakklankdistorsie by die onderskeie groepe. om die akoestiese identifikasie van die aard van spraakklankdistorsie moontlik te maak, word die volgende betroubare en akoesties identifiseerbare spraakparameters van normale sprekers, sprekers met serebellere disartrie en sprekers met verbale apraksie telkens ondersoek en vergelyk: die spraakparameter stemaanvangstyd (sat) soos gemeet tydens die produksie van 'n stemhebbende eksplosiewe klank, aangesien stemaanvangstyd 'n aanduiding verskaf van die sinchronisasie van glottale sluiting en supraglottale artikulasie (tyler & waterson, 1991). die spraakparameter afsluitingsduur soos gemeet tydens die produksie van 'n stemhebbende eksplosiewe klank, aangesien afsluitingsduur temporale aspekte van spraakproduksie, soos die spoed van artikulatoriese beweging'en ruimtelike aspekte soos die omvang en akkuraatheid van artikulasiebewegings reflekteer. die spraakparameter konsonantduur soos gemeet tydens die produksie van 'n frikatiewe klank en 'n kontinuante klank, aangesien konsonantduur temporale aspekte van spraakproduksie soos die spoed van artikulatoriese bewegings reflekteer. die spraakparameters formantfrekwensiewaardes van die eerste, tweede en derde formante van 'n kontinuante klank, aangesien formantwaardes ruimtelike aspekte van spraakproduksie soos die omvang en akkuraatheid van artikulatoriese bewegings reflekteer. die spraakparameter aard van die energiespektrum soos gemeet tydens die produksie van 'n frikatiewe klank, aangesien frekwensie-omvang en plek van gekonsentreerde energie ruimtelike aspekte van spraakdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 88 isna erasmus, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald produksie soos die omvang en akkuraatheid van artikulasiebewegings reflekteer. proefpersone kriteria vir die seleksie van proefpersone en kontrolepersone die volgende algemene kriteria is gestel: die persone se taalbegrip, ouditiewe begripsvermoens en visuele vermoens moet van so 'n aard wees dat die persoon die instruksies verstaan en die materiaal kan lees. die proefpersone moet nie 'n binourale gehoorverlies van groter as 12% he nie aangesien 'n groot gehoorverlies 'n invloed op spraak kan he. proefpersone met serebellere disartrie: die proefpersone moet deur 'n neuroloog gediagnoseer wees as breinbeseerd met serebellere skade. die neuromotoriese aantasting moet so suiwer as moontlik tot die serebellum beperk wees. die proefpersone moet disartriese spraak vertoon. die sprekers moet gepaardgaande kenmerkende liggaamlike neuromotoriese simptome toon ter bevestiging van serebellere skade bv. ataksie, dismetrie, disdiadokokinese, intensietremor en hipotonie. proefpersone met verbale apraksie: die proefpersone moet deur 'n neuroloog gediagnoseer wees as breinbeseerd met 'n fokale letsel wat nie 'n serebellere lokalisasie het nie. die proefpersone moet verkieslik 'n suiwer verworwe verbale apraksie vertoon en indien 'n gepaardgaande afasie voorkom mag dit slegs minimaal wees. die proefpersone moet voldoen aan kriteria soos gestel deur kent en rosenbek (1983) nl: * onvlot spraakproduksie met probeer-en-tref artikulasiebe wegings. * onkonstante foutproduksie by herhaalde produksie van dieselfde woord. * disprosodie. * probleme met inisiering van uitinge. die graad van apraksie moet die herhaling van uitinge moontlik maak. geen gepaardgaande disartriesimptome mag voorkom nie. kontrolepersone: die kontrolepersone moet normaalsprekende volwassenes wees wat geen spraakafwykings of geskiedenis van spraakof neurologiese afwykings het nie. die kontrolepersone moet: van dieselfde geslag as die proefpersone wees; binne die ouderdomsgrens van die proefpersone val; moet dieselfde spreektaal as die proefpersone he. geselekteerde proefpersone en kontrolepersone proefpersone met serebellere disartrie: twee sprekers met verworwe serebellere disartrie wat voldoen aan die vasgestelde vereistes is gevind. proefpersoon 1 (pp 1) is 'n manlike spreker wat na 'n motorongeluk in 1979 algemene geringe breinskade en ernstige serebellere skade opgedoen het. hy toon tans neuromotoriese liggaamlike simptome soos ataksie, dismetrie, hipotonie, disdiadokokinese en intensietremor. proefpersoon 2 (pp 2) is 'n manlike spreker wat ongeveer in 1989 ligte tekens begin toon het van balansprobleme. die oorsaak van die aantasting is in maart 1992 gediagnoseer as serebellere degenerasie weens veelvuldige sklerose. veelvuldige sklerose neem meestal oorsprong in die serebellum (bannister, 1973). alhoewel veelvuldige sklerose geleidelik 'n gemengde neurologiese toestand tot gevolg kan he, toon pp 2 slegs kenmerkende liggaamlike neurologiese simptome van 'n serebellere aard. proefpersoon 2 se algemene graad van aantasting blyk groter te wees as die van pp 1, aangesien pp 2 'n erger graad van liggaamlike simptome vertoon as pp 2 en ook meer disartries voorkom. proefpersoon met verbale apraksie: een persoon met verworwe verbale apraksie wat voldoen aan die vasgestelde vereistes is gevind. die spreker met verbale apraksie is 'n manlike spreker, proefpersoon 3 (pp 3), wat 'n gepaardgaande minimale broca afasie vertoon (aq=64) volgens die western aphasia battery (kertesz, 1982). proefpersoon 3 het verworwe breinskade opgedoen as die resultaat van twee serebro-vaskulere insidente onderskeidelik in 1984 en 1989. infarksies het voorgekom in die anterior been van die linker interne kapsula en in die regter oksipitale gebied. die spreker toon 'n regsydige hemiplegie maar daar is geen aanduiding van 'n waarneembare boonste motorneuron gesigof tongparese nie. proefpersoon 3 toon verbale apraksie in 'n erge graad maar is in staat tot nabootsing van woorde. tydens spraak kom duidelike soekbewegings en worstelgedrag voor, asook pogings tot selfkorreksie, onkonstante distorsies en vervangings, en onkonstante prodiiksies met die herhaling van uitinge. ; ι i ι i materiaal | ι die materiaal is so gekies dat dit die omvattende ruimtelike en temporale ondersoek van spraakklank-, distorsie in 'n verskeidenheid van kontekste moontlik maak, om sodoende 'n meer verteenwoordigende beeld te verkry van spraakproduksie by die onderskeie diagnostiese groepe. die materiaal is saamgestel deur die kontinuante klank [1], die stemhebbende eksplosiewe klank [d] en die frikatiewe klank [s] te varieer in die inisiele en mediale klankposisies van onsineenhede en woorde,binne sinsverband met 'n kvkven kvkvk-struktuur onderskeidelik. die klanke is gekies omdat dit verskillende wyses van artikulasie verteenwoordig en akoesties maklik identifiseerbaar is. die vokaal [a:] is telkens met die klanke in die onsineenhede gekombineer en is soos die ander klanke in die woorde en sinne lukraak gekies. twee verskillende klankstrukture is gekies om so spraakthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spraakklankdistorsie by neuromotoriese spraakafwykings 89 klankdistorsie in materiaal van verskillende uitinglengte te ondersoek. die materiaal bestaan uit 12 onsineenhede en 10 woorde binne sinsverband. die onsineenhede is telkens 5 keer herhaal en die sinne drie keer. in totaal het elke persoon dus 90 uitinge geproduseer. tabel 1 gee 'n volledige uiteensetting van die wyse van die samestelling van die teikenmateriaal en die gemete spraakparameters binne elke uiting. tabel 1. samestelling van die teikenmateriaal en die gemete spraakparameters binne elke uiting 1 . 1 klank [d] konteks klankkombinasie klankposisie parameters materiaal onsinwoorde kvkv inisieel sat data onsinwoorde kvkv mediaal ad sada onsinwoorde kvkvk inisieel sat dadaf onsinwoorde kvkvk mediaal ad sadaf sinne kvkvk inisieel sat dames sinne kvkvk mediaal ad dadel 1.2 klank [s] konteks klankkombinasie klankposisie 1 parameters materiaal / / onsinwoorde inisieel kd fo piek sasa / / onsinwoorde kvkv mediajal kd fo piek dasa / / onsinwoorde inisieel kd fo piek sasaf / / onsinwoorde kvkvk media'al 1 kd fo piek dasaf sinne inisieel kd fo piek sabel sinne ivvjvvk / 1.3 klank [i] konteks klankkombinasie klankposisie parameters materiaal onsinwoorde kvkv insieel kd f1-f3 lala onsinwoorde kvkv mediaal kd f1-f3 dala onsinwoorde kvkvk inisieel kd f1-f3 lalaf onsinwoorde kvkvk mediaal kd f1-f3 dalaf sinne kvkvk inisieel kd f1-f3 laken sinne kvkvk apparaat opname-apparaat 'n nakamichi 550 "versatile stereo casette system". 'n bever dynamic μ 201 n(c) mikrofoon. basf c h r o m e maxima 60 m i n u t e m a g n e e t bandkasette. analise-apparaat 'n nakamitchi 550 kassetspeler is gebruik om die spraaksein soos opgeneem op basf chrome maxima magneetbandkassette na die kay dsp sonagraph te stuur. die spraaksein kan met behulp van die jbl pro iii luidsprekers gemonitor word. die spraaksein word geanaliseer deur 'n digitale seinprosesseerder van kay elemetrics corp. nl. sonagraph model 5500 en vertoon op 'n nec multisync ii vertoonskerm. drie verskillende toepaslike opstellings is vir die onderskeie analises gebruik. 'n weergawe van die beeld wat op die skerm verskyn kan met behulp van 'n termiese drukker, kay dsp drukker model 5510 verkry word. prosedure vir spraakseinopname die spraakopnames is in die klankdigte opnamelokaal van die taallaboratorium van die universiteit van pretoria gedoen. die mikrofoon is op 'n afstand van 10 cm voor die persoon se mond geposisioneer om ekspirasiegeraas op die opname tot die minimum te beperk. die materiaal is vooraf aan die persone gegee om deur te lees sodat hulle bekend kon raak daarmee. die volgorde van die materiaal is konstant gehou vir al die sprekers. die proefpersone is gevra om elke woord vyf keer direk na mekaar te herhaal en elke sin drie keer. vir die verbaal-apraktiese spreker is die opname effens gewysig deurdat die proefpersoon die geleentheid gegee is om die uiting 'n paar keer te oefen (kent & rosenbek, 1983). daar is deurentyd 'n opname gemaak totdat vyf produksies van elke uiting verkry is. analise van data analise van die spraakparameter stemaanvangstyd (sat) vir die meting van sat is 'n analise-opstelling met 'n gekombineerde vertoning bestaande uit 'n golfvorm met 'n amplitudeverloop en 'n grondtoonverloop sowel as 'n spektrogram gebruik. by die bepaling van satwaardes is die twee vertonings voortdurend vergelyk om sodoende die betroubaarheid van metings te verhoog. 'n positiewe stemaanvangstyd word gedefinieer as die tyd wat verloop vanaf die aanvang van die vrylating van ploffing tot by die aanvang van die eerste die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 90 isna erasmus, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald vertikale striasie op die spektrogram wat glottale pulsering verteenwoordig (lisker en abramson, 1964). sat-waardes le op 'n kontinuum en kan positiewe en negatiewe waardes aanneem. in die geval van 'n negatiewe sat-waarde gaan glottale pulserings die ploffing vooraf en word die meting gedoen vanaf die aanvang van glottale pulserings tot by die ploffingsgedeelte. analise van die spraakparameter afsluitingsduur afsluitingsduur is gedefinieer as die periode in millisekondes wat verloop vanaf die einde van die voorafgaande vokaal tot voor die aanvang van die ploffing van die eksplosief. wanneer daar nie aanduidings van 'n volledige afsluiting waarneembaar was op die golfvorm nie, is die spektrogram gebruik ter vergelyking om so betroubaarheid van metings te verhoog. analise van die spraakparameter-konsonantduur vir die meting van konsonantduur is analise-opstellings gekies wat gelyktydig 'n spektrogram, 'n gekombineerde golfvorm en 'n gemiddelde amplitudespektrum van 'n spesifieke gedeelte van 'n uiting vertoon. die frekwensieomvang van die analise-opstellings het verskil nl. 0-4 khz vir die [1] en 0-16 khz vir die [s]. konsonantduur is gedefinieer as die periode in millisekondes (msek) wat 'n konsonant spektrografies waarneembaar is. analise van die eerste, tweede en derde formantfrekwensiewaardes van die [i] vir die meting van die formantfrekwensiewaardes is dieselfde frekwensie-omvang gekies as wat gebruik is vir die analise van konsonantduur van die [1] (nl. 0-4 khz). die tydkursors op die spektrogram is so geplaas dat dit 'n verteenwoordigende stabiele deel van die [1] afbaken en 'n gemiddelde amplitudespektrum daarvan verkry. met behulp van frekwensiekursors is die formantfrekwensiewaarde vanaf die gemiddelde spektrum bepaal deur die tweede energiepiek as f l , die derde piek as f2 en die vierde piek as f3 te noteer. die gemiddelde amplitude-spektrum en spektrogram is deurentyd vergelyk. analise van die omvang van energie en piek van gekonsentreerde energie van die [s] vir die meting van die omvang van akoestiese energie en piek van gekonsentreerde energie van die [s] is dieselfde opstelling gebruik as by konsonantduur (d.w.s. 0-16 khz). met behulp van tydkursors is 'n verteenwoordigende deel van die [s] op die spektrogram afgebaken en frekwensiekursors geplaas op die onderste en boonste grense. die piek van gekonsentreerde energie is bepaal deur die energiepiek met die hoogste waarde op die amplitudespektrum te identifiseer. waar twee ewe sterk pieke waargeneem is, is die waarde van die tweede piek van energie genoteer. verwerking van data verwerking van sat-metings vir die berekening van hierdie sat-foutwaarde is satwaardes van -180 millisekondes tot +15 millisekondes (msek) aanvaar as die normale perke (zlatin, 1974). die gemiddelde sat-foutwaarde vir elke groep uitinge van elke persoon is bereken deur die hoeveelheid millisekondes bokant 15 of onder 180 oor die drie herhalings bymekaar te tel en deur drie te deel (van der merwe et al., 1989). hierna is 'n gemiddelde sat-foutwaarde vir elke persoon bereken. 'n gemiddelde sat-foutwaarde vir die onderskeie diagnostiese groepe is vervolgens bereken uit die gemiddelde foutwaardes van die betrokke proef en konrolepersone. verwerkings van afsluitingsduurmetings van die [d] en konsonantduurmetings van die [i] en [s] die resultate van afsluitingsduurmetings van die [d] en konsonantduurmetings vir die [1] en die [s] is telkens op dieselfde wyse verwerk. eerstens is 'n gemiddelde duurwaarde vir die eerste drie herhalings van elke onsineenheid en elke woord van elke persoon bereken. uit die gemiddeldes van elke persoon is tweedens 'n gemiddelde duurwaarde vir elke onsineenheid en elke woord vir die betrokke diagnostiese groepe bereken. uit die gemiddelde duurwaardes van die verskillende onsineenhede is derdens 'n totale gemiddelde duurwaarde vir onsineenhede bereken vir elke diagnostiese groep en vierdens op dieselfde wyse 'n totale gemiddelde duurwaarde vir woorde vir elke diagnostiese groep. laastens is 'n gemiddelde duurwaarde bereken vir elke diagnostiese groep, uit die som van die onderskeie gemiddelde waardes vir onsineenhede en woorde tesame. verwerkings van die resultate van formantfrekwensiewaardes van die [i] die gemete formantfrekwensiewaardes van die eerste drie formante van die [1] is telkens op dieselfde wyse verwerk. eerstens is daar vir elke persoon gemiddelde formantfrekwensiewaardes vir formante een, twee en drie bepaal uit die som van die onderskeie gemete formantfrekwensiewaardes vir onsineenhede,en woorde tesame. tweedens is gemiddelde formantfrekwensiewaardes vir elke formant van elke diagnostiese groep bepaal uit die som van die betrokke gemiddelde formantfrekwensiewaardes van die persone. derdens is forma'ntratio's bepaal vir elke persoon en elke diagnostiese groep vir formant twee teenoor formant een (f2:f1) en vir formant drie teenoor formant een (f3:f1) volgens die metode van dalston (in baken en daniloff, 1991). verwerking van die resultate van omvang van akoestiese energie en die piek van gekonsentreerde energie van die [s] / / vir die bepaling van die omvang van akoestiese energie is die gemiddelde mininmm en maksimum frekwensiewaardes van elke persoon en elke diagnostiese groep bereken. die gemiddelde minimum en maksimum frekwensiewaardes van elke persoon is bereken uit die the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spraakklankdistorsie by neuromotoriese spraakafwykings 91 som van die gemete onderskeie minimum en maksimum frekwensiewaardes van onsineenhede en woorde tesame. uit hierdie persoongemiddeldes is vervolgens gemiddelde minimum en maksimum frekwensiewaardes vir die onderskeie diagnostiese groepe bepaal. persoonen diagnostiese groepgemiddeldes is verder op dieselfde wyse bereken vir die piek van gekonsentreerde energie van die [s], resultate en bespreking resultate en bespreking van stemaanvangstydmetings die resultate van hierdie spraakparameter dui op verskille in die aard van die waargenome spraakklankdistorsie by die onderskeie afwykings. figuur 1 illustreer die gemiddelde sat-foutwaardes van die verskillende diagnostiese groepe. die verbaal-apraktiese spreker toon die grootste foutwaarde (nl.14.8) van die verskillende groepe. gemiddelde sat-foutwaarde 14 12 normaal czh 3fb*m9 oturu· • • v«fbat* apimksw s.o. diagnostiese groepe v.a. figuur 1. gemiddelde sat-foutwaardes van die ' onderskeie diagnostiese groepe. / i / ι ontleding van die sat-data toon dat die serebellere disartriesprekers se sat-resultate konstant afwykend negatiewe sat-waardes toon by pp 2 terwyl pp 1 se waardes rondom die normale perke sentreer. teoreties beskou impliseer 'n negatiewe sat-waarde dat fonasie 'n aanvang neem voordat die lugvrylating vir afsluitingsklankproduksie plaasvind (lisker & abramson, 1964). produksie van die [d] vereis hoofsaaklik integrasie van produksiekomponente soos stemgewing, velere en alveolere sluiting om die opbou van lugdruk moontlik te maak en verder verbreking van die alveolere afsluiting met gevolglike ploffingsproduksie. by nadere ondersoek blyk dit egter dat die gemete negatiewe sat-waardes en waarneembare stemvoorloop by pp 2 die resultaat is van η perseptueel hoorbare nasalering van die aanvangsklank asook swak afsluitings en ploffingsproduksie w;at meting bemoeilik het (sien resultate aangaande afsluitingsduurmetings vir verdere toeligting). stemgewing by pp 2 is dus wel akkuraat in tyd, maar blyk skynbaar afwykend negatief te wees weens genasaleerde aanvangsdistorsie van die [d], omdat duidelike afsluitingsproduksie nie plaasgevind het nie. dit blyk dus asof die aanvang van die ploffingsgedeelte vertraag is, moontlik weens swak velere sluiting as gevolg van'hipotonie of onwillekeurige bewegings. daar is dus sprake van swak interartikulatorsinchronisasie weens stadige velere sluiting en nie weens stadige inisiering van stemgewing nie. die resultate dui dus moontlik op intakte beplanning van die artikulatoriese bewegings maar op probleme met die uitvoer van die beplande bewegings. die feit dat pp 1 nie soortgelyke afwykende satresultate vertoon as pp 2 nie, is moontlik 'n aanduiding dat die sat-afwykings by die serebellere disartriesprekers direk verband hou met die erns van die aantasting. kent et al. (1979) vind ooreenstemmende resultate deurdat satwaardes van klanke soos die [t], [p] en [k]-klanke verleng word namate die graad van aantasting toeneem. die verbaal-apraktiese spreker in die huidige studie toon in teenstelling met die serebellere disartriesprekers 'n hoe sat-foutwaarde en ook slegs sat-waardes van 'n positiewe aard. die positiewe sat-waarde dui daarop dat glottale pulserings (d.w.s. fonasie) 'n aanvang neem nadat die ploffing van die afsluiting vir die [d] geproduseer is. die vertraging in stemgewing by pp 3 reflekteer moontlik probleme met die temporale interartikulator-sinchronisasie van tongbeweging en glottale sluiting wat 'n spektrografies waarneembare distorsie van die [d] as byna 'n [t] tot gevolg het. aangesien die sat-waardes van die spreker in hierdie studie nooit die gegewe positiewe satwaarde van +40 msek (zlatin, 1974) vir stemlose klanke bereik het nie ('n gemiddelde sat-waarde van 26.6 msek kom voor), kan die afleiding gemaak word dat die spreker nie die [d] fonologies vervang het met 'n [t] nie en dat 'n spraakklankdistorsie eerder teenwoordig is. soortgelyke afleidings is deur van der merwe et al. (1989) gemaak. die sat-resultate van die verbaal-apraktiese spreker in die huidige studie stem ooreen met bevindinge van freeman, sands en harris (1978) naamlik dat sprekers met verbale apraksie geen stemvoorlope vir stemhebbende afsluitingsklanke het nie. navorsing toon oor die algemeen dat stemhebbende klanke meer stemloos gemaak word deur verbaal-apraktiese sprekers as omgekeerd (wertz, lapointe en rosenbek, 1984). kent en rosenbek (1983) vind in teenstelling met huidige bevindinge sat-foute van 'n groot negatiewe aard by sommige van hul proefpersone en wys daarop dat probleme met koordinasie en tydsberekening akoesties verskillend manifesteer by verskillende verbaal-apraktiese sprekers. daar kan gespekuleer word dat verbaal-apraktiese sprekers moontlik ideosinkratiese aantasting van sinchronisasie van fonasie en artikulasie vertoon en dat sat-foute, alhoewel dit voorkom by verbaal-apraktiese sprekers, onvoorspelbaar is in terme van die aard van die sat-foute (freeman et al., 1978; van der merwe et al., 1989). daar moet egter in gedagte gehou word dat slegs data van een verbaal-apraktiese spreker in die huidige studie beskikbaar is, wat dus spekulasies hieromtrent aansienlik beperk. resultate van afsluitingsduurmetings figuur 2 toon dat die serebellere disartriesprekers se gemiddelde afsluitingsduurwaarde (nl. 0.09 sek) gering korter is as die van die normale sprekers (nl. 0.1 sek) terwyl die verbaal-apraktiese spreker die langste afsluitingsduurwaarde vertoon (nl. 0.49 sek). dis insiggewend dat die produksie van 'n swak alveolere afsluiting vir die [d], waarneembaar is by beide serebellere disartriesprekers. spektrografiese ontleding toon dat feitlik geen onderbreking in glottale pulserings met die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 92 isna erasmus, anita van der merwe & emily groenewald 0.θ 0.7 0.θ" 0.5 0.4 0.3 0.2 0.1 afsluitingsduur (sek.) q«m. in onelneenhede i / j gem. in woord· γπ qxnkkmcl· duui η normaal s.d. v a diagnostiese groepe figuur 2. gemiddelde afsluitingsduurwaardes van die [d] van die onderskeie diagnostiese groepe, soos bereken vir onsineenhede, woorde en vir woorde en onsineenhede tesame. aanduidings van 'n ploffing waarneembaar is op die golfvorm nie, terwyl die spektrogram ook 'n mate van aaneenlopendheid in die formante vertoon. die aaneenlopende teenwoordigheid van glottale pulserings en energie op die spektrogram bemoeilik dus die bepaling van die afsluitingsgedeelte van die [d] in die woord en het tot gevolg dat verkorte tot normale afsluitingsduur gemeet word by die serebellere disartriesprekers. hierdie tendens dra dus by tot die gemete negatiewe sat-waardes by veral pp 2. proefpersoon 2 se sat-waardes was gevolglik in twee van die totaal van nege uitinge nie meetbaar nie, weens die feit dat 'n duidelike ploffing vir die [d] nie teenwoordig was nie. die resultate en afleidings van die huidige studie aangaande swak afsluitingsklankproduksie by die serebellere disartriesprekers word ondersteun deur die resultate van vorige studies. kent en netsell (1975) vind konstante afsluitingsklankdistorsie van die [t]-, [d]en [g]-klanke deur serebellere disartriesprekers deurdat die afsluitingsgedeelte van die klank of weggelaat word, (waarskynlik waarneembaar as 'n vokaalagtige distorsie), of met 'n frikatiewe kwaliteit geproduseer word. hulle rapporteer ook 'n afwesigheid van 'n prominente ploffing van die [g]-klank en skryf dit toe aan 'n onvermoe van die tong om kontak te maak met die alveolere rif weens onvoldoende spierkrag of onwillekeurige bewegings. hierdie resultate dui moontlik op probleme op 'n uitvoervlak van spraakproduksie. dis egter ook moontlik dat die voorafgaande resultate uitvalle op 'n programmeringsvlak van spraakproduksie kan reflekteer, waartydens daar sprake is van "spier-spesifieke" programmering van omvang en rigting van spraakbewegings (van der merwe, 1994). in teenstelling met die serebellere disartriespreker in die studie (pp 2) toon die verbaal-apraktiese spreker (pp 3) spektrografies beskou 'n duidelike afsluitings of ploffingsgedeelte maar stemgewing en ploffing word "afsonderlik" geproduseer. fonasie neem dus 'n aanvang nadat die afsluiting opgehef is. hierdie resultate dui daarop dat daar nie probleme voorkom met die kwaliteit van afsluitingsproduksie by die verbaal-apraktiese spreker nie. verlengde afsluitingsduurwaardes by verbaalapraktiese sprekers word ook deur ander navorsers gerapporteer (kent & rosenbek, 1983; hardcastle et al., 1985). verlenging van afsluitingsduur by die verbaalapraktiese spreker kan moontlik verklaar word as 'n kompensatoriese reaksie om sodoende meer tyd toe te laat vir die motoriese beplanning van die daaropvolgende bewegingskomponente van die uiting (kent & rosenbek, 1982; 1983). hierdie verklaring word moontlik ondersteun deur die waarneming in die huidige studie dat die verbaalapraktiese spreker langer afsluitingsduurwaardes vir kvkvk-eenhede as vir kvkv-eenhede vertoon. navorsing aangaande die invloed van verskillende kontekste op die spraak van verbaal-apraktiese sprekers dui daarop dat die kvkvk-eenheid vir 'n verbaal-apraktiese spreker moeiliker is om te beplan as die kvkv-eenheid (van der merwe etal., 1988; 1989). langer afsluitingsduurwaardes sou dan moontlik meer tyd toelaat vir die spreker om die uiting te beplan. resultate en bespreking van konsonantduurmetings van die [1] en die [s] figure 3 en 4 illustreer die resultate van konsonantduurmetings. die serebellere disartriesprekers en die verbaal-apraktiese spreker toon volgens die resultate ongeveer dieselfde mate van temporale distorsie by die [l]-klank, deurdat die mate van verlengde kontinuantduur min of meer gelyk is (sien figuur 3). vir die [s]-klank toon die verbaal-apraktiese spreker egter 'n groter mate van temporale distorsie as die serebellere disartriesprekers. die serebellere disartriesprekers toon volgens die konsonantduur (sek.) gem. in onelneenhede i / j gem. in woord· [π gemiddelde duur 0.08 0.08 μ μ μ 01 01 01 01 υ// υ// υ// υ// υ// s.d. diagnostiese groepe figuur 3. gemiddelde konsonantduurwaardes van die [1] van die onderskeie diagnostiese groepe. konsonantduur (sek.) η gem. in onsmeenhede i / j gem. in woord· i i i gemiddelde duur ys.d. · diagnostiese groepe figuur 4. gemiddelde frikatiewe duurwaardes vir die [s] van die'onderskeie diagnostiese groepe. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spraakklankdistorsie by neuromotoriese spraak resultate verlengde konsonantduur vir beide die [1] en die [s]-klanke. verlengde segmentele duur by sprekers met serebellere disartrie is ook deur ander navorsers bevind (kent & netsell, 1975; kent et al., 1979; kent & rosenbek, 1982). die huidige resultate stem ooreen met die van kent et al., (1979) wat verlengde konsonantduur ooreenkomstig die graad van aantasting vind by serebellere disartriesprekers. temporale distorsie in die vorm van verlengde konsonantduur by die serebellere disartriesprekers kan eerstens verklaar word as die direkte gevolg van neuromotoriese simptome soos hipotonie, ataksie en onwillekeurige bewegings wat 'n afname in die spoed van bewegings kan veroorsaak (grunwell & huskins, 1979) en dus moontlik probleme op 'n uitvoervlak van spraakproduksie reflekteer. tweedens kan verlengde duur beskou word as die resultaat van kompensasie vir die onderliggende simptome, om sodoende meer tyd toe te laat vir die evaluasie van terugvoer aangaande die posisies en bewegings van die artikulators (kent & rosenbek, 1982). kent et al., (1979) noem in aansluiting by so 'n beskouing dat wanneer sprekers met serebellere disartrie genoeg tyd tot hulle beskikking het, hulle toepaslike artikulatoriese posisies kan bereik. in die huidige studie vertoon pp 1 duurwaardes langer as die normale vir die [1] maar by die frikatiewe [s]-klank kom distorsie voor in die vorm van duurwaardes korter as die normale. kent et al., (1979) rapporteer ook duurwaardes kleiner as die normale vir die [s]-klank vir persone met minder ernstige serebellere disartrie maar verskaf geen ander verklaring daarvoor as die graad van aantasting nie. daar kan gespekuleer word dat pp 1 moontlik as gevolg van 'n unieke kompensasiewyse spraak vinniger as die normale produseer of dat vinniger as normale [s]-produksie 'n unieke spraakkenmerk van die persoon was voordat die verworwe spraakafwyking ingetree het. formele metings van spraakspoed en uitinglengte is egter nie beskikbaar ter verdere uitbreiding van die spekulasies rondom spraakspoed nie. metings van die akoestiese omvang van energie van die [s] het egter meer lig gewerp op die artikulasie-eienskappe van die [s] (sien verdere bespreking). j die verbaal-apraktiese spreker toon ook soos die serebellere disartriesprekersjtemporale distorsie van die [s] maar verskille in die aard en graad daarvan is opgemerk. 'n groter mate van temporale spraakklankdistorsie kom voor by die [s] van die verbaalapraktiese spreker deurdat duurwaardes groter is as die waardes van die serebellere disartriesprekers en die normale waardes (sien figuur 4). hierdie resultaat stem ooreen met algemene bevindinge dat verbaal-apraktiese sprekers verlengings van woordsegmente toon (kent & rosenbek, 1983; kent & mcneil, 1987; van der merwe et al., 1989). navorsers soos kent en mcneil (1987) voer aan dat verlengde segmentele en intersegmentele duur by verbaal-apraktiese sprekers die direkte resultaat kan wees van foutiewe fonetiesmotoriese kodering van die uiting. ander outeurs beskou weer verlengde segmentele duur teoreties as 'n kompensatoriese metode (kent & rosenbek, 1982; van der merwe et al., 1989). 'n interessante spektrografiese waarneming by duurmetings van die verbaal-apraktiese spreker is die konstante voorkoms van intersillabe-pouserings, selfs waar normale duurwaardes bereik is. in teenstelling hiermee het intersillabe-pouserings nooit by die serebellere disartriesprekers voorgekom nie en is aaneenlopende i n g s 93 produksies sonder duidelike afslui tings eerder opgemerk. kent en mcneil (1987) vind in ooreenstemming met hierdie resultate intersegmentduur langer as die normale by verbaal-apraktiese sprekers. hierdie bevindinge vind dus tot 'n mate aansluiting by die teorie dat intersillabepouserings as 'n kompensatoriese metode gebruik word. 'n verdere interessante waarneming wat by die duurmetings van die [s] sowel as die [l]-klank van die verbaal-apraktiese spreker waarneembaar is en nie by die sprekers met serebellere disartrie voorkom nie, is dat die verbaal-apraktiese spreker gering groter duurwaardes vir kvkvk-eenhede vertoon as vir kvkv-eenhede. van der merwe et al., (1989) vind ooreenstemmende groter duurwaardes vir kvkvk-eenhede as vir kvkv-eenhede by verbaal-apraktiese sprekers. kontekste wat moontlik hoer eise aan die spreker stel in terme van motoriese beplanning het dus moontlik verlengde duur tot gevolg (van der merwe et al., 1989). die afleiding kan dus gemaak word dat die verbaal-apraktiese spreker in die huidige studie ook moontlik probleme vertoon met die beplanning van spraak. resultate en bespreking van die meting van die omvang van akoestiese energie van die [s]-klank figuur 5 illustreer die bevindinge van die meting van die omvang van akoestiese energie van die [s]-klank. 1 2 3 4 6 8 7 8 9 1 0 11 12 frakwenaie ( k h z ) figuur 5. gemiddelde minimum en maksimum frekwensie-waardes en piek van gekonsentreerde energie vir die [s] van die verskillende diagnostiese groepe. die moontlikheid dat unieke [s]-produksie by ppl voorkom, word verder ondersteun deur resultate van die meting van die omvang van akoestiese energie by die [s] van ppl. die serebellere disartriesprekers as groep vertoon ruimtelike distorsie van die [s] deurdat die omvang van die [s] skuif na 'n effens kleiner waarde met die minimum waarde meer afwykend as die maksimum waarde (sien figuur 5). 'n verlaging in die minimum frekwensiewaardes van die [s] kan moontlik dui op 'n verminderde konstriksie vir [s]-produksie. proefpersoon 1 toon 'n gering groter omvang van akoestiese energie as pp 2 wat daarop kan dui dat 'n groter mate van [s]-distorsie voorkom by pp 1 ten spyte van normale en korter as normale duurwaardes. proefpersoon 1 toon dus 'n groter mate van ruimtelike spraakklankdistorsie vir die [s]-klank as pp 2, terwyl pp 2 weer 'n groter mate van temporale spraakklankdistorsie vir die [s] vertoon. die omvang en akkuraatheid van die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 40, 1993 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 94 artikulatoriese bewegings is dus moontlik by die s t n n : r r i e r p r e k e r s ^ ^ ^ — e n k o l i d graad van neuromotoriese simptome daar ^ die voorkoms van spraakklankdistorsie * sen*el,ere disartriesprekers, d e ^ d i e n ^ ng van akoestiese energie heelwat wyer is as die o m z g van x normale en serebellere disartriesprekers (sien ^ u r 5 ) η afname in die algemene intensiteit van die ui c k o m ook voor, wat spektrografies waarneembaar s at ^ akoestiese energie kan die gevolg wees van 'n z ^ ^ mg ν k o n s t r i k s i e t y d j apraktiese spreker vertoon dus moontlik probleme met die fyn posisionering van die a r t i l r n l ^ p r o m e , m e m e t gekanaliseerde lugstroom te verseke^ kent ^ (1983) postuleer l p r o ^ z t t ^ z s u , ? waar min,f«n 4. , teorie van motoriese kontrole waar ruimtelik-temporale skemata "or abstract a g e e q u i v a l e n t in y e a r s figure 2. profile of age equivalent functioning on the metalinguistic test battery. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 the results for the word analysis subtest (segmentation of bisyllabic words) are compared with results obtained with normal three to six year olds in figure 4. the three oldest subjects achieved scores of 100% which places them on a six year old level. subject c scored on a thrge to four year level and subject β on a level below three years. the literature from which the task was drawn (fox & routh, 1975) found that most children made use of conventional syllable boundaries to break up the wgrds, which was also the case for subjects c, d and e, who scored 100% for this task (see figure 2). subject f, however, segmented 7/8 of the stimuli by giving only the initial phoneme which reflects ability to analyse words on a sub-syllable level, but not sensitivity to syllabic boundaries. the rhyme subtest, (figure 2) which assesses phonological awareness, had a criteria of 90% to determine mastery of the skill. only subject ε met the criteria, with subjects b, c and f on a four year level and subject g below a three year level. discussion the results of this research project support the hypethesis that metalinguistic (or language awareness) abilities in mentally handicapped children do not necessarily develop spontaneously as they do with normal x ο 3 ο u j ο: ο ο cη π χ / ζ , χ c d b f — s c o r e s o b t a i n e d w i t h n o r m a l 3 t o 5 y e a r o l d s i > 4 5 6 a g e in y e a r s figure 3. comparison of scores for sentence analysis with normal 3 to 5 year olds. a x ο c ο 6 tx i ° 4 u j a: ο u η t/> 2 χ χ s c o r e s o b t a i n e d w i t h n o r m a l 3 t o 6 y e a r o l d s 4 5 6 a g e in y e a r s figure 4. comparison of scores for word analysis with normal 3 to 6 year olds. debra marais & erna alant children, which is also the case with learning disability. the fact that these language awareness skills are lacking, makes it plausible that the difficulty many mentally handicapped children experience in learning to read could be contributed, at least in part, to inadequate metalinguistic skills. many mentally handicapped people derive benefit from training in other metacognitive skills. this makes it seem possible that this p o p u l a t i o n could benefit from t r a i n i n g in metalinguistic skills. word segmentation skills were well developed in three subjects, i.e., on a six to seven year old level. these abilities contrasted sharply with poor sentence segmentation abilities in all but one of the subjects. poor performance on this task was shown despite the nature of the task which gave much opportunity for learning to take place (van kleeck et al., 1987). one subject even gave responses which broke phrases up at sub-word boundaries, and others gave perseverative responses after the sentence had been broken up into two or three phrases. three of the five subjects yielded scores which fell below scores attained by three year olds. thus the awareness of the word as the basic unit of speech was clearly not well established. the discrepancy found between word and sentence analysis abilities in mentally handicapped children contrasts sharply with results obtained for the same two tasks in normal children. the research from which the tasks were drawn (fox & routh, 1975; smith & tagerflusberg, 1982) found such close correlations between these two abilities that they postulated that the same cognitive process underlies both skills. some could argue that this difference between mentally handicapped and normal children is merely another reflection of the differences between the two populations, and that comparisons between them are of little value. however, if the factor of reading instruction is introduced into the discussion, another possibility is raised. all the children who obtained high scores of word segmentation had received formal reading instruction, much of which focusses on word and phonemic analysis. they made use of word analysis and synthesis skills throughout the testing, and seemed proud of this ability wliich they had mastered. it could be the case that these high scores were a reflection of training in isolated metalinguistic skills, rather than a reflection of development of metalinguistic abilities. since some learning hald taken place, despite the discrete nature of the skills, this again strengthens the case raised in the first sectibn that mentally handicapped children can benefit from training in metalinguistic skills. a last point to consider with regard to sentence analysis abilities is the strong correlation that has been found between the ability to segment sentence into words and reading performance. evans et al., (1979) and mcnich (1974 in van kleeck & schuele, 1987) and tunmer et al., (1984) found awareness of aural word boundaries to be a significant predictor of early reading achievement. this ability was poorly developed in all but one of the subjects. subject e, who had mastered this skill (see figure 4), was also the only subject who was functionally literate. thus it would seem that awareness of aural word boundaries had a similar correlation with reading performance in the mentally handicapped population as well. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the development of precursors to literacy in mentally disabled children 21 conclusion the mentally handicapped children used in this study displayed inadequate and incomplete development of the metalinguistic precursors to literacy which were investigated, despite these children having language abilities comparable to a six year old age level, good literacy socialisation environments and receiving formal reading instruction. trends which were noted in all subjects could be indicative that the difficulties uncovered here are difficulties that many mentally handicapped children experience. this metalinguistic deficit could be due to the difficulty that many mentally handicapped children have with meta-cognitive abilities, and could also be a contributing factor to the difficulty that this population experiences with reading acquisition. more research using larger sample groups is needed to establish this. it is also possible that reading acquisition could be enhanced by training metalinguistic skills which are valuable for reading, since splinter skills had been developed in some of the subjects. since training these skills has become part of the speech pathologist's role in intervention with language disabled children (van kleeck & schuele, 1987), speech pathologists may need to play a role in training metalinguistic skills in the mentally handicapped population as well, in order to improve literacy in this population. references berger, r.s. & reid, d.k. (1989). differences that make a difference : comparisons of metacomponential functioning and knowledge bases among groups of high and low iq learning disabled, mildly mentally handicapped and normally achieving adults. journal of learning disabilities, 22(7), 422-429. cherkes-julkowski, m. & gertner, n. (1989). spontaneous cognitive processes in handicapped children. berlin : springer report. edmiaston, r.k. (1988). preschool literacy assessment. seminars in speech and language, 9(1), 27-36. flood, j. & salus, m. (1982). metalinguistic awareness : its role in language development and its assessment. topics in language disorders, 2 (sept). fox, b. & routh, d.k. (1975). analysing spoken language into words, syllables and phonemes: a developmental study. journal of psycholinguistic research, 4(4), 331-342. garton, a. & pratt, c. (1989). learning to be literate the development of spoken and written language. cornwell : basil blackwell ltd. t.j. press, 218-221. goodenough, f.l. (1926). the measurement of intelligence by drawings. london : harrap. gouws, k.l. (1975). voorlopige vertaling van die peabody picture vocabulary test item ontleding met die oog op standaardisasie. unpublished blog paper, university of pretoria. kahmi, a. g. & koenig, l.a. (1985). metalinguistic awareness in language disordered children. language, speech and hearing services in schools, (july), 199-210. marais, d. (1991). metalinguistic precursors to literacy in mentally retarded children. unpublished b.log. paper, university of pretoria. reid, d.k. (1981). child reading : readiness or evolution. topics in language disorders. special issue on language in context : listening, reading and writing, 61-71. schuele, m.c. & van kleeck, a. (1987). precursors to literacy: assessment and intervention. topics in language disorders, 7(2), 32-44. sher, k. (1989). the relatedness of auditory receptive language and visual receptive language in preschool learning disabled children. unpublished b.a. log paper, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. singh, n.n. & singh, j. (1986). reading acquisition and remediation in the mentally retarded. international review of research in mental retardation, 14, 165-193. smith, c.l. & tager-flusberg, h. (1982). metalinguistic awareness and language d e v e l o p m e n t . journal of experimental child psychology, 34, 449-468. tunmer, w.e. & pratt, c. & herriman, m.l. (eds.), (1984). metalinguistic awareness in children. berlin : springel-verlag. van kleeck, a. (1990). emergent literacy : learning about print before learning to read. topics in language disorders, 10(2), 25-45. van kleeck, a. & schuele, c.m. (1987). precursors to literacy: normal development. topics in language disorders, 7(2), 13-31. vorster, j. (1980). handleiding vir die toets vir mondelinge taalproduksie (tmt). pretoria suid-afrikaanse instituut vir psigometriese navorsing. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) v ^ u m m u i m i c a i i u i m a i d s for people who cannot speak dual displays o n e for t h e user & o n e for t h e p e r s o n t h e y a r e s p e a k i n g t o single switch plug-in single s w i t c h e s for s c a n o p e r a t i o n m e m o r y store f r e q u e n t l y u s e d phrases k e y b o a r d splash-proof a n d either q w e r t y or a b c d e l a y o u t the sl35 lightwriter t h e l i g h t w r i t e r r a n g e o f a l t e r n a t i v e c o m m u n i c a t i o n a i d s a r e p o r t a b l e , l i g h t w e i g h t a n d e x t r e m e e a s y t o o p e r a t e . s i m p l y t y p e w h a t y o u w a n t t o s a y a n d t h e u ^ h f f i slh s a y i t τ ο γ τ ο ω ^ ^ b ' e ^ ° v q r i e t y ° f s i n 9 ' e s w i t c h e s " ^ ™ w ^ o a r e l l i n h ! l i ! ! r r s ? k ° v e a „ r a n g e ° f h i g h q u a l i t y s p e e c h s y n t h e s i s e r s i n c l u d i n g d e c t a l k t h i sl35 i n c . u d a a l l e s t c ™ i c a t i o n a i d t h e w o r l d w i t h d e c t a l k s p e e c h s y n ^ l s f e a t u r e s i ' m ? m ™ e d , c ! i o o n " b a s e d ° n 4 0 0 0 m o s t g e n t l y s p o k e n english w o r d s . l i 9 h — *> « a p o r t a b l e w o r d processor ϊ 1 8 ® ^ " ° f s p a c e a f t e r c o m m a e t c ' t o s a v e typing tremor c o n t r o l ant,-tremor d e l a y to p r e v e n t i n a d v e r t e n t o p e r a t i o n with p o o r h a n d c o n t r o l users sole distributor o f t o b y churchill lightwriters a tw τ γ ρ a t ) ρ ο box 8 4 6 3 5 g r e e n s i d e 2 0 3 4 l / \ h j telephone: (011) 486-09-70 m i c r o s y s t e m s f a c s i m i l e : ( o i l ) 6 4 6 1 5 8 0 specialist suppliers of lightwriters a n d a d a p t e d computer e q u i p m e n t for special needs r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) editorial for the first time we are able to include in our journal, an article in afrikaans, which was made possible by the co-operation of the staff of the department of logopedics and speech science of the university of pretoria. we sincerely hope that this will be the first of many such articles. several of the articles indicate that numerous avenues for further research do exist in south africa. the success of a journal of this nature depends upon the extent to which its members take an active part in contributing to it. the journal aims to reflect the positive growth and development of logopedics and its allied fields in south africa. we must therefore draw on the resources of south african speech therapists and the allied medical professions for our material. we hope that our readers will find this issue of the journal of the south african logopedic society of interest. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) redaksioneel vir die eerste keer is dit moontlik om 'n artikel in afrikaans in die tydskrif te plaas. ons is hiertoe instaat gestel deur die samewerking van die personeel van die departement spraakheelkunde by die universiteit van pretoria. ons hoop dat dit die eerste van nog vele sulke artikels sal wees. verskeie artikels in hierdie tydskrif beklemtoon die behoefte aan verdere navorsing in suid-arika. die sukses van 'n tydskrif van hierdie aard hang uitsluitlik af van die aktiewe belangstelling van die lede en die bydraes wat hulle lewer. die doel van hierdie tydskrif is om die positiewe groei en ontwikkeling van logopedika en die verwante gebiede in suid-afrika aan te dui.daarom. ook, is ons afhanklik van bydraes deur suid-afrikaanse spraakterapeute en die verwante professies. ons hoop dat die lesers van hierdie uitgawe van die tydskrif van die suidafrikaanse spraakheelkundige vereniging interessant sal vind. journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) editorial there are many speech therapists who are aware of the value of the bobath techniques in the handling of the cerebral palsied child, but the article by jacobs and shapiro fills that vital gap between theory and practice in that they give a detailed step by step account of the application of the method. they also discard some older techniques and point out pitfalls to avoid in therapy. in his article on the aphasic child, myklebust goes into the details of every term which he uses thus establishing a clear frame of reference in which to discuss the problem. through his warm and understanding approach we begin to see the brain-injured child as a troubled little human and not merely as a theoretical concept. we are indeed priveleged to be able to publish this contribution from one of the outstanding workers in this field. surgical procedures for the relief of deafness due to otosclerosis are appraised by d. r. haynes. we thus obtain some appreciation of the problem from the point of view of the medical man. in this issue therefore, we have presented a detailed discussion and new information on topics that are of interest and importance to all workers in speech therapy and related fields. june, 1961 journal of the south african logopedic society 3 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 25 early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: a transdisciplinary model of service delivery legini moodley, brenda louw and rene hugo department of communication pathology university of pretoria abstract traditional models of service delivery prevent speech-language therapists and audiologists from identifying and implementing early communication intervention (eci) in south africa. this study utilized the framework of the transdisciplinary team approach to develop a collaborative partnership between community nurses and speech-language therapists and audiologists. an interdisciplinary in-service training programme was implemented with 24 community nurses employed at family health clinics in kwa-zulu natal; with the aim of enhancing their knowledge and attitudes toward the early identification and referral of at-risk children to speech-language therapists and audiologists. three questionnaires were used to evaluate the nurses' knowledge and attitudes. results indicated that the training programme significantly improved the nurses'knowledge regarding eci but not their attitudes. implications of these findings are discussed in terms of service delivery for eci, and the education and training of early interventionists in the south african context. key words: early communication intervention, infants plinary in-service training. introduction early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers is the first crucial step in early communication intervention (eci), and it has a significant impact on treatment efficacy. a factor that is consistently linked to the efficacy of early /intervention services is that of age ofidentification (rossetti, 1993; 1996). improving age of identification by implementing methods of service delivery that promote earlier detection of children!who are at-risk for developing communication disorders is important for effective management, and constitutes an important goal of speechlanguage therapists and audiologists. however, gaining earlier access to infants and toddlers and their families in need of eci appears to be an international and national challenge confronting speech-language therapists and audiologists. despite research evidence to support early intervention, specifically eci, many children with communication disorders are not referred to speech-language therapists and audiologists as the prevailing attitude of many health professionals continues to be "wait and see as the child may outgrow it" (rossetti, 1996; louw & kritzinger, 1991). this attitude reflects the need for greater consultation and collaboration between speech-language therapists and audiologists and referring health care professionals to promote greater awareness of speech, language and audiological services. delayed communication development is identified as the most common symptom of developmental disability in and toddlers, at-risk, transdisciplinary team model, interdiscichildren under three years of age, affecting approximately 5% to 10% of that population (rossetti, 1996). in south africa, it is estimated that children under the age of four years comprise 5 million of the country's 38 million population (a national health plan for south africa, 1994; central statistical services, personal communication, july 2, 1998). using rossetti's (1996) estimates, which are relevant for a developing country, south africa could expect approximately 250 000 to 500 000 children to present with delayed communication development. the prevalence of developmental delays and disabilities in children under three years of age is rapidly increasing due to advances in life-saving medical technology and new and expanding populations of infants and toddlers (rossetti, 1996). in developing countries like south africa, at-risk children and their families face additional political, social, cultural, economic, linguistic, and environmental conditions that place them at increased risk for developmental disabilities (pickering et al., 1998). the white paper on integrated national disability strategy (1997) indicates that 80% of black children with disabilities live in extreme poverty in inhospitable environments. the implication for speech-language therapists and audiologists is evident: early identification of communication disorders in infants and toddlers (0-3years), and the timely provision of early communication intervention (eci) is vital to enhance the overall developmental potential of these children. there are currently only 1750 registered speechlanguage therapists and audiologists (y. hoffman, health die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 26 legini moodley, brenda louw and κβηέ hugo professions council of south africa, personal communication, 21 august 2000) to cater for the country's approximately 3,8 million people who require communication treatment (pickering et al., 1998). however, a very small number of these professionals are adequately trained in eci (fair & louw, 1999). besides the shortage of qualified personnel, speechlanguage therapists and audiologists' use of traditional institution-based models of service delivery have proven to be ineffective in reaching the majority of the vulnerable and disadvantaged communities of south africa. the restructured national health system mandates a transition in service delivery from institution-based services to community-based services (white paper on integrated national disability strategy, 1997). the south african government has adopted the primary health care (phc) approach as the underlying philosophy for the restructuring of its health system (department of health, 1995). however, besides legislating free health care services for all children under six years of age, specific policies for the disabled and at-risk population are lacking (white paper on integrated national disability strategy, 1997; fair & louw, 1999). the principles underlying south africa's national health system provide strong support for the implementation of recommended eci practices. asha (1989) identified four quality indicators for effective eci services to the at-risk population, namely; services need to be family-centred, community-based, comprehensive, and co-ordinated. moreover, research has identified the team model of service delivery as being critical for the provision of comprehensive and coordinated eci services (asha, 1989; fair & louw, 1999). the transdisciplinary team model of service delivery is currently viewed as the preferred model of practice in early intervention (briggs, 1997). it is an integrative service delivery model that is characterized by three operational principles, namely; role release, role expansion, and arena evaluation as it requires professionals across disciplines to pool and exchange information, knowledge, and skills by crossing and re-crossing traditional boundaries. the latter principles provide a mechanism for the delivery of community-based, family-centered, comprehensive and coordinated eci services to at-risk infants and toddlers. the transdisciplinary model is ideal for the diverse south african context as it supports the phc and community-based models of service delivery. speech-language therapists and audiologists practice at the tertiary level of health care, whilst the at-risk population is present at the primary level. the adoption of the transdisciplinary principles will allow speechlanguage therapists and audiologists to establish partnerships with referring health professionals (based at the primary level of health care), thereby facilitating access to infants and toddlers who are at-risk for developing communication disorders (fair & louw, 1999). moreover, asha (1991) states that the transdisciplinary model is especially suited to service contexts where the demand is incongruous with the services provided because of the shortage of qualified staff, and when economic and other factors restrict service provision. in light of the current south african context and the suitability of the transdisciplinary team model for eci, this model of service delivery was selected to enable south african speech-language therapists and audiologists to gain earlier access to children at-risk for developing communication disorders. for this study, community nurses employed at community-based family health clinics were selected as the health professionals with whom speech-language therapists and audiologists could consult and collaborate. community-based primary health care nurses are the frontline health professionals in the early intervention team as they have direct contact with at-risk children, and are based at family health clinics that are accessible and affordable to the majority of the south african population. more importantly, in contrast to the current 1750 registered speech-language therapists and audiologists, there are approximately 21000 qualified community nurses (national health plan, 1994). in addition, infants receive developmental screening at family health clinics from six weeks of age, thus presenting the ideal context for the monitoring of early communication development. effective collaborative partnerships require both partners to share a common philosophy about the outcome of their services (briggs, 1997); and to possess a specialized core of knowledge and skills for working with at-risk infants, toddlers and their families (roberts, crais, layton, watson, & reinhartsen, 1995). however, current research findings indicate that many health professionals do not possess this common philosophy or specialized knowledge and skills (weitzner-lin, chambers & siepierski, 1994; delport, 1998). one of the ways to ensure that community nurses possess the necessary attitudes and knowledge to identify and appropriately refer children at-risk for communication disorders, is for the speech-language therapist and audiologist to serve as a source of information and provide relevant in-service training programmes to community nurses (rossetti, 1993; delport, 1998). although the transdisciplinary team approach provides the infrastructure to promote mutual teaching and learning, it does not facilitate the process of education and training, thus necessitating the adoption of an interdisciplinary model of education and training. interdisciplinary education refers to the collaboration of two or more disciplines in the learning process (american association of colleges of nursing, 1996), and is identified as the recommended practice for the education and training of early interventionists (briggs, 1997). l b facilitate the training process and the identification of community nurses' specialized knowledge and attitudes for eci, a model of the role of community nurses in eci was developed. this model identified the primary roles of community nurses in eci as being: screening, identification, referral and monitoring of at-risk children. based on these roles, the theoretical framework proposed by hanson and brekken (1991) was utilized to identify the nurses' specialized knowledge and attitudes for eci. this framework stipulates that early interventionists' specialized attitude and knowledge base is developed from two levels: (1) a set of professional discipline-specific knowledge and attitudes; and (2) a common early intervention knowledge and attitude base. figure 1 illustrates the development of the three levels of knowledge from discipline specific to specialized. however, will an interdisciplinary in-service training programme influence community nurses' knowledge and attitudes regarding eci, so that it promotes their ability to identify and refer children who are at-risk for communication disorders ? this is the research question that formed the basis for the present study. according to briggs (1997) training programmes provide high quality, low-cost and easily accessible opportunities for professionals to master new service delivery roles; while also ensuring that infants, toddlers, and their families have appropriate and effective the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: eci services. the provision of training programmes in eci by speech-language therapists and audiologists to community nurses, is poorly documented in the literature (delport, 1998). consequently, the aim of this study is to determine whether an in-service training programme for community nurses will influence their knowledge and attitudes with respect to the early identification of at-risk children. method aim the purpose of this study was to determine whether an inservice training programme for community nurses employed at family health clinics influenced their knowledge and attitudes with respect to the early identification of infants and toddlers (0-3 years) who were at-risk for developing communication disorders. the following objectives were formulated: • determination of community nurses' present knowledge and attitudes with regard to the early identification of infants and toddlers (0-3 years) who are at-risk for developing communication disorders. ransdisciplinary model of service delivery 27 • development and implementation of an in-service training programme to enhance community nurses' knowledge and attitudes with regard to the early identification of infants and toddlers (0-3 years) who are at-risk for developing communication disorders • determination of the effect of the in-service training programme on community nurses' knowledge and attitudes with regard to the early identification of infants and toddlers (0-3 years) who are at-risk for developing communication disorders. research design an exploratory descriptive survey quasi-experimental time series design was utilized (rosnow & rosenthal, 1996). this design involved utilizing the survey method to obtain a measure of the knowledge and attitudes of community nurses with regard to eci. this evaluation was then followed by the implementation of an in-service training programme. thereafter, the survey method was utilized again to obtain two further measures of the nurses' knowledge and attitudes following the training programme, one immediately after the training programme and the second 6 weeks later. specialized knowledge and attitude for early communication intervention level 3 knowledge: * specific risk factors for communication disorders * importance of communication domain in relation to other developmental domains * concept of continuum of risk * role of speech-language therapist and audiologist * screening assessment of infant and toddlers' communication skills * criteria for identifying the presence or absence o f a communication disability * speech-language therapy and audiology resources in the community * basic communicationbased intervention attitude: a positive attitude towards the importance of communication skills for overall development, and the need for early referral to the speech-language therapist and audiologist. t common knowledge and attitude regarding early intervention level 2 knowledge: * legislation on early intervention (international and national directives) * efficacy o f early intervention * epidemiologic overview o f the at-risk inlant and toddler population * team model of service delivery, with specific reference to the transdisaplinaiy team approach attitude: a positive attitude towards early intervention, its' effectiveness, and the benefit of the transdisciplinary team model of service delivay. discipline-specific knowledge and attitude for early communication intervention level 1 knowledge: * normal infant development * atypical infant development * family dynamics and assessment * planning, implementing and evaluating an intervention plan * community's medical, social and financial resources a ttitude: a positive attitude towards the management o f infants, toddlers and their families. t figure 1: the three levels of knowledge that community nurses require for practice in early communication intervention. (conceptualized from hanson & brekken, 1991; rossetti, 1996; brandt & magyary, 1989; and cox, 1996). i die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 subjects the target population for this study was community nurses employed at family health clinics, which are situated in the durban functional region of kwa zulu natal. the subjects were required to meet the following criteria. community nurses needed to be registered with the south african nursing council to ensure that they had a valid qualification in nursing, since the study focused on their knowledge and attitudes with regard to eci. they also needed to be currently employed at family health clinics, and involved in the practice of developmental monitoring of infants and toddlers as their knowledge and attitudes in this area was the focus of the study. selection of community nurses nonprobability convenience sampling was utilized (leedy, 1993), which entailed the researcher working with those community nurses who were selected by the nursing administrators. therefore, this method of sampling made legini moodley, brenda louw and hugo no pretence of being representative of the population; and did not attempt to control for bias (leedy, 1993). logistical constraints imposed by the need to maintain service delivery at the clinics, allowed only 30 of the total 127 community nurses to participate in the study description of the community nurses in the study on the day of data collection, only 24 community nurses participated in the training programme, representing 19% of the total population. all the nurses in the sample completed and returned the pre-training and post-training 1 survey (i.e. questionnaires 1 and 2), thereby representing a 100% response rate. however, only 21 of the 24 nurses completed and returned the post-training 2 survey (i.e, questionnaire 3). the response rate for questionnaire 3 was therefore 88%. table 1 provides the biographical characteristics of the subjects who participated in the pre-training and two posttraining surveys of the study. table 1: biographical characteristics of the community nurses who participated in the three surveys characteristics pre-training survey and post-training survey 1 n=24 pre-training survey and post-training survey 1 % post-training survey 2 n=21 post-training survey 2 % work environment family health clinic 24 100% 21 100% geographic area durban functional region 24 100% 21 100% professional qualification degree diploma 5 19 21% 79% 3 18 14% 86% community nursing qualification yes no 23 1 96% 4% 20 1 95% 5% employment status permanent full-time 24 24 100% 100% 21 21 100% 100% ! experience at family health clinics 0-5 years 6-10 years 11-15 years 16-20 years >20 years 14 6 1 2 1 58% 25% 4% 8% 4% 12 6 1 2 0 1 57% 29% 5% 10% 00% attendance at early communication intervention workshop/training • programme yes no 0 24 0% 100% 21 0 100% 0% first language english zulu 16 8 67% 33% 15 6 76% 4% the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: materials and apparatus three questionnaires and an in-service training programme were the main materials and apparatus that were utilized in the study. self-administered, questionnaires were used as they facilitated the observation of the nurses' knowledge and attitudes regarding eci, which were beyond the physical reach of the researcher (leedy, 1993). questionnaire 1 was utilized in the pre-training survey, questionnaire 2 was utilized in the post-training 1 survey, while questionnaire 3 was utilized in the post-training 2 survey, as reflected in figure 2. the design and development of the questionnaires guidelines provided by brink (1996); rosnow and rosenthal (1996) and leedy (1993) were used to develop the questionnaires. the questionnaires comprised four main sections, with an additional section included in questionnaires 2 and 3, which focused specifically on the nurses' evaluation of the training programme. the four sections were: (a) biographical information; (b) discipline specific knowledge for eci; (c) common knowledge and attitudes for early intervention; and (d) specialized knowledge and attitudes for eci. [refer to tables 1, 3, 4, 5, and 6 for information regarding questions included in each section]. all questions, except the last one, were constructed using the structured closed-ended format, in order to accommodate for the time constraints of the nurses. the questionnaires contained content that was relevant for community nurses to know, in order to implement their roles of screening, ransdisciplinary model of service delivery 29 identifying, referring and monitoring children who are atrisk for communication. [for detailed motivations regarding the questions that were included in the three questionnaires, the reader is referred to the original study by moodley, 1999], reliability and validity of the questionnaires reliability was ensured by paying careful attention to the formulation, length and complexity of questions; pilottesting the questionnaire; coding of data; and qualitatively analyzing the items in the questionnaire by providing an estimate of the internal consistency of the instrument (brink, 1996). a thorough and extensive review of the literature ensured that only relevant items were included. some of the questions were based on the questionnaire utilized by louw & weber (1997), and to ensure content validity, the questionnaires were approved by two south african experts in the field of eci. design and development of the in-service training programme the training programme was designed to accommodate the restrictions placed by durban city health department regarding minimal disruptions in service delivery. consequently, it was planned to be two hours in duration and to occur over two afternoons (1 hour per afternoon). table 2 presents a description of the development of the training programme. table 2: development of the training programme factors use in training programme aims philosophy structure j 1 i implementation process training environment evaluation strategy • to enhance community nurses' knowledge in eci • to foster community nurses' development of positive attitudes towards eci • to promote a community-based, family-centred, comprehensive and co-ordinated philosophy towards the rendering of eci services to at-risk infants and toddlers (asha, 1989). • an outcomes-based approach was utilized as hugo (1996) states that training that is based on outcomes provides a logical correlation between education and professional practice. accordingly, the guidelines of the national qualifications framework were used to develop specific outcomes; and information by bennet, watson & raab (1995) were used to develop a training plan (refer to figure 2) • the following five factors informed the implementation process: a systems approach to change was adopted in order to effect longterm practice changes (briggs, 1997). a preliminary needs analysis was conducted to ensure that training was responsive to the perceived needs of the nurses (winton, 1996). an empowerment perspective was adopted to create the necessary conditions for change (trivette, dunst, hamby & lapointe, 1996). a choice of instructional strategies were utilized that supported the principles of adult learning, for example, participant choice and responsibility for own learning (winton, 1996). follow-up support was provided to ensure application of learning (winton, 1996) • the training offices of the durban city health department. • analysis of the nurses' responses on the pre-training questionnaire (questionnaire 1) and the two post-training questionnaires (questionnaires 2 and 3). key: eci = early communication intervention ι die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 validity and reliability of the in-service training programme content validity was established by presenting the plan of the training programme to two expert early interventionists with experience in the use of training programmes, as well as the nursing administrators for their critical review (rosnow & rosenthal, 1996). since the training programme was implemented over two afternoons, every effort was made to ensure that similar conditions prevailed during the implementation process for both sessions. trainer bias was controlled for by adherence to a strict legini moodley, brenda louw and hugo training protocol (rosnow & rosenthal, 1996). in addition, a pilot study of the programme was conducted. pilot studies on the questionnaires and in-service training programme pilot studies of the three questionnaires and the training programme were conducted to increase the accuracy and reliability of the data. four community nurses, who were not part of the main study, were selected according to the criteria stipulated earlier. on the basis of the pilot studies the questionnaires and training programme were adapted. | introduction and orientation to the research process pre-training survey questionnaire 1 ph^se: implementation of training programme background to the study of eci: problems in early identification of at-risk children • definition and epidemiological overview of at-risk children • early intervention and eci • legislation on early intervention • efficacy of early intervention proposed solution : collaboration and the transdisciplinary approach • importance of the team approach to service delivery in south africaspecifically the transdisciplinary team approach • best practice for ei in south africa: community-based, lamily-centered, comprehensive and coordinated service part 1 phase 3 3 role of south african community nurses in eci • screening assessment, identification, referral and monitoring • information that will promote implementation of the above roles: risk factors for communication problems concept of continuum of risk early communication behaviours to monitor, and factors to consider during screening assessment criteria for identifying the presence of a communication disorder role of the speech-language therapist and audiologist speech-language therapy and audiology resources in the community to facilitate early referral guidelines on basic communication-based intervention to assist with education and counselling of the caregiver part 2 post-training survey 1 questionnaire 2 post-training survey 2 questionnaire 3 figure 2: an illustration of the implementation of the main study the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: a transdisciplinary model of service delivery 31 the main study the main study was conducted over three phases, namely; the pre-training survey, implementation of the inservice training programme, and the post-training surveys. figure 2 illustrates the processes of the three phases as they occurred. during the introduction the trainer did not indicate her professional background, nor was any information regarding early intervention or eci presented at this stage so as not to influence the nurses responses on the pre-training survey. the pre-training survey occurred first and the trainer was present to answer queries thereby preventing misunderstandings from biasing the data (brink, 1996). the training programme commenced immediately after questionnaire 1 was collected. part 1 of the training programme commenced on the first afternoon with a brief discussion of the background to the study of eci, and ended with the proposed solution of the transdisciplinary approach (refer to figure 2). part 2 of the training programme continued the next afternoon and focussed on the role of south african community nurses in eci. a handout on eci, prepared on the basis of the training plan, was distributed to the nurses at the start of the programme to facilitate the learning process. at the end of part 1 of the training programme, the nurses were allowed to take the handout with them and were encouraged to read it. immediately after part 2 of the training programme ended, questionnaire 2 was distributed and collected. six weeks after the training programme, questionnaire 3 was given to the nursing administrators, for distribution to the participants of the training programme, and the completed questionnaires were collected from the nursing administrators. data analysis descriptive and inferential statistical methods were employed. data were analysed using the statistics computer software called statistical package for the social sciences (spss) [leedy, 1993], descriptive statistics, which included frequency counts, percentages, and measures of central table 3: community nurses' level of confidence in th preand post-training surveys tendency and variance, were used to describe and summarise the data (rosnow & rosenthal, 1996). inferential statistics, namely; the parametric one-way analysis of variance (anova) [rosnow & rosenthal, 1996] was utilized to determine whether any differences found among the three evaluations, were real differences that may be attributed to the influence of the training programmes. when a statistically significant difference was obtained, post-hoc comparison tests utilizing the bonferroni test, were' calculated to determine which of the means differed significantly (brink, 1996). thematic analysis was utilized to analyse the open-ended questions. results and discussion the results are discussed in accordance with the main aim of the study. the findings for objectives one and three are presented concurrently to highlight any similarities and differences that may exist among the three measures. influence of the in-service training programme on community nurses' knowledge and attitudes with regard to early communication intervention the findings are presented in accordance with the framework presented in figure 1, namely; the community nurses' discipline-specific·, common and specialized knowledge and attitudes regarding eci. specialized knowledge and attitudes are presented according to the nurses' four roles in eci, namely; screening, identification, referral and monitoring. community nurses'discipline-specific knowledge for early communication intervention results indicate that the training programme did influence the community nurses'discipline-specific knowledge with respect to eci. anova analysis was significant at the 0.01 level, while post-hoc comparisons found that the means for questionnaire 1 and 2 differed significantly. table 3 reveals that before training, the majority of the nurses ir discipline-specific knowledge areas as evident in the knowledge area not confident % moderately confident % confident % ql q2 q3 ql q2 q3 ql q2 q3 1. normal infant development 4.2 0 4.8 25 8.3 23.8 70.8 91.7 71.4 2. atypical infant development 16.7 0 4.8 58 25 42.9 25 75 52.4 3. family functioning and assessment 4.2 0 9.5 54.2 29.2 38.1 41.7 70.8 52.4 4. assessment of infants and toddlers 16.7 4.2 0 41.7 33.3 33.3 41.7 62.5 66.7 5. planning, implementing and evaluating an intervention plan 20.8 4.2 0 54.2 29.1 47.6 25 66.7 52.4 6. knowledge of the community's medical, social and financial resources 25 0 9.5 j 37.5 4.2 28.6 37.5 95.8 61.9 key: ql = questionnaire 1 q2 = questionnaire 2 q3 = questionnaire 3 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 legini moodley, brenda louw and hugo expressed moderate levels of confidence in their disciplinespecific knowledge, especially in the areas relating to atypical infant development, family functioning and family assessment, and planning, implementing and evaluating an intervention plan. however, immediately after training a significantly large number of the nurses felt confident, while six weeks after the training programme, although most of the nurses still expressed confidence, a large number indicated moderate levels of confidence. it is concluded that overall, more nurses were moderately confident rather than confident in their discipline-specific knowledge base. community nurses' discipline-specific knowledge base represents the foundation from which they would develop a common early intervention knowledge base and a specialized eci knowledge base (hanson & brekken, 1991). despite the positive trends, however, it is concerning that the pre-training survey found most of the nurses to be moderately confident rather than confident in their discipline-specific knowledge base. the nature of the nurses' pre-service education and training experiences may provide an explanation as cox (1996) states that the compressed and streamlined nursing curricula and the shortage of suitably qualified teaching staff are possible factors that may impact on the quality of early intervention education at the pre-service level. informal discussions with community nurses in kwa-zulu natal revealed that their pre-service training was also compressed and streamlined to equip them with the broad-based knowledge and skills that they required as primary health care providers..it appears that the nurses possess an unsatisfactory level of discipline-specific knowledge for eci, to facilitate their competent implementation of eci at family health clinics. community nurses' common knowledge and attitudes regarding early intervention common knowledge regarding early intervention: table 4 indicates that the training programme did influence the nurses' common knowledge for early intervention, as the anovaanalysis was statistically significant at the 0.05 level, and the bonferroni test found questionnaire 3 to be significant. the nurses presented with adequate levels of common knowledge with respect to the definition, efficacy, and principles of early intervention, on all three questionnaires, although training resulted in a slight increase in knowledge levels. the nurses' knowledge of international legislation increased significantly six weeks after training. unfortunately, similar findings did not occur with respect to their knowledge of south african legislation, as inadequate knowledge levels were evident on all three evaluations. it is important for community nurses to be knowledgeable about the four areas of early intervention, as this will play a significant role in influencing their attitudes toward eci, as well as facilitate effective service delivery to at-risk children and their families (brandt & magyary, table 4: community nurses' common knowledge regarding early intervention as evident during the preand post-training phases knowledge area % true % uncertain % false knowledge area qi q2 q3 qi q2 q3 qi q2 q3 1. early intervention is a range of health care services that is available to promote the well being of families with infants and toddlers who maybe at-rick for developmental disabilities 95.8 100 100 4.2 0 0 0 0 0 2. international laws do not authorize the provision of early intervention services to at-risk infants and toddlers. 4.2 33.3 4.8 41.7 8.3 9.5 54.2 58.3 85.7 : 1 3. south african health care laws authorize the provision of early intervention services to at-risk infants and toddlers. 87.5 62.5 61.9 8.3 4.2 4.8 4.2 33.3 ί 33.3 4. early identification and initiation of intervention services for developmentally delayed children is not effective in reducing and/or preventing later delay. 0 0 0 8.3 4.2 0 91.7 95.8 100 5. community-based, family-centered, coordinated and comprehensive services will promote effective management of at-risk children 95.8 100 100 4.2 0 0 0 0 0 key: q1 = questionnaire 1. q2 = questionnaire 2. q3 = questionnaire 3. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: a transdisciplinary model of service delivery 33 1989). the community nurses' demonstration of an adequate common knowledge base with respect to three of the four areas on the pre-training survey, strongly suggests the possibility that the information is currently being provided in their discipline-specific training. this finding has relevance for the planning of eci in-service training programmes for community nurses, as these areas may not need to be addressed in detail in future programmes (delport, 1998). however, the nurses' limited knowledge regarding international and national legislation on early intervention makes it clear that the nurses' common knowledge base for early intervention is inadequate. the nurses' response to the question regarding south african legislation on early intervention may be attributed to the government's legislation of free health care services for all children under six years of age (department of health, 1995). be this as it may, however, specific and detailed policies with respect to the provision of early intervention services for the at-risk infant and toddler population as outlined by the international legislation relating to public law 99-457 (rossetti, 1996), is lacking in south africa. the latter is currently identified as a priority issue in the white paper on integrated national disability strategy (1997). it is clear that the current assumption that the nurses possess this common knowledge is incorrect, and supports research findings that many health professionals receive poor educational preparation for their roles as early interventionists (weitzner-lin et al., 1994; delport, 1998). common attitudes regarding early intervention: the training programme did not influence the nurses' common attitudes as the anova analysis was not significant at the 0.05 level. the findings in table 5 generally indicate that the nurses expressed positive attitudes toward using a team approach to service delivery in early intervention. nonetheless, some of them appear to have reservations about the transdisciplinary concepts of role release and role expansion. community nurses' possession of a common attitude base will facilitate the development of a philosophy towards eci that is shared by speech-language therapists and audiologists (rossetti, 1996). the current finding indicates that the nurses commenced the training programme with a positive attitude toward team management. similar findings were obtained in the recent south african studies by louw and weber (1997) and delport (1998). discussions with the south african nurses during the training programme revealed that they were adequately informed of the importance of teamwork during their pre-service training. they also indicated familiarity with the workings of the multidisciplinary and interdisciplinary team models, but not the transdisciplinary model. it is important to note that the transdisciplinary approach is a relatively recent model of team functioning that is discussed in the literature (briggs, 1997), and this may account for the nurses' unfamiliarity with the model. the nurses' reservations regarding the principles of role release and role expansion may indicate their fear of the loss of professional autonomy and the blurring of professional boundaries, as discussed by briggs (1997). the need for the nurses to be informed of the benefits of transdisciplinary practice for eci is clear, as the short duration of the current training programme may not have been sufficient in this regard. table 5: community nurses' attitudes regarding teamwork in early intervention as evident during the preand post-training phases / / / attitudes toward teamwork % true % uncertain % false attitudes toward teamwork ql q2 q3 ql q2 q3 ql q2 q3 1. a team approach to service delivery is important for effective management of at-risk infants and toddlers. 100 100 100 0 0 0 0 0 0 2. health care professionals should be encouraged to share their discipline-specific knowledge and skills by training other health care professionals. 95.8 95.8 95.2 4.2 0 0 0 4.2 4.8 3. health care professionals should be encouraged to carry out the roles and responsibilities of other professionals, provided that they are trained to do so. 70.8 79.2 71.4 4.2 4.2 4.8 25 16.7 23.8 4. it is important for community nurses and speech-language therapists and audiologists to consult each other about children who are at-risk for developing communication disorders. 100 100 100 0 0 0 0 0 0 key: q l = questionnaire 1. q2 = questionnaire 2. q3 = questionnaire 3. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 legini moodley, brenda louw and hugo table 6: community nurses' specialized knowledge and attitudes regarding early communication intervention community nurses roles in eci specialized knowledge regarding eci specialized attitudes regarding eci screening results per question: dl: table 7 reveals that the nurses' pre-training knowledge regarding the importance of communication for overall development of the child was limited. increased knowledge was evident after training. d2: pre-training knowledge was adequate as > 90% of the nurses considered a communication problem to be a factor that could influence the emotional, cognitive and social development of a child d4: table 8 indicates that the nurses' pre-training knowledge regarding the 13 risk factors was inadequate, but improved after training though not in all. anova: significant at 0.01 level bonferroni test: significant differences between means for questionnaires 1 and 2, and questionnaires 1 and 3. conclusion: training programme did influence community nurses specialized knowledge for screening results per question: before and after the training programme, the nurses expressed positive attitudes towards the assessment of communication skills (d3), and the reliability and validity of the developmental screening assessment utilized at the clinics (d5&6). however, shifts in the nurses' responses across the three conditions of testing indicate some degree of confusion regarding the reliability and validity of the screening assessment. anova: not significant at 0.05 level conclusion :training programme did not influence the nurses' specialized attitudes towards screening identification d7: pre-training knowledge was adequate with respect to factors that adversely affect the results of a communication assessment. a further improvement was noted after training. d8: the nurses were not confident about their knowledge of the eligibility criteria that need to be applied when identifying a child as being at-risk for developing a communication disorder. confidence levels increased significantly after questionnaire 2 (70.8%), but decreased to 57.2% on questionnaire 3 d9: excellent pre-training knowledge of the symptoms of a communication disorder anova: not significant at 0.05 level conclusion: training programme did not influence the nurses' specialized knowledge regarding the identification of children at-risk for communication disorders. referral and monitoring d10: the nurses displayed excellent knowledge in 6 of the 7 roles of the speech-language therapist and audiologist before and after training. knowledge regarding the management of feeding problems appeared to be problematic, even after training. d13: before and after training, the majority indicated that they would refer at-risk children to the speech-language therapist and audiologist d15: pre-training knowledge about the community's speechlanguage therapy and audiology resources was poor, but increased significantly after training. d16: pre-training confidence in the nurses' knowledge regarding eci was inadequate, but improved significantly after training. anova: significant at 0.01 level bonferroni test: significant differences between the means for questionnaire 1 and 3 and questionnaires 1 and 2 conclusion: training programme did influence community nurses' specialized knowledge for referral and monitoring d l l : preand post-training responses supported their roles in eci as identified in the study however, displayed some confusion regarding the depth of the assessment required d12:displayed positive attitudes toward the early referral of at-risk children before and after training d14:preand post-training responses indicated positive attitudes, toward the beneficial effects of speech-language therapy and audiology services for at-risk children 1 d17: overwhelming support for receiving assistance from speech-language therapists and audiologists in the form of inservice training programmes anova: not significant at 0.05 level conclusion: training programme did not influence community nurses' specialized attitudes toward referral and monitoring overall conclusion : the training programme did influence community nurses' specialized knowledge for eci overall conclusion: the training programme did not influence community nurses'specialized attitudes toward eci key: eci = early communication intervention. dl-17: refer to questions in the questionnaires the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: a transdisciplinary model of service delivery 35 community nurses' specialized knowledge and attitudes regarding early communication intervention table 6 provides a summary of the main findings of the nurses' specialized knowledge and attitudes for each of the four eci roles described. overall findings signify that the community nurses in this study did not appear to possess the specialized knowledge that is required to enable them to effectively execute their tasks in eci. this finding, once again, supports current research literature which contends that many health professionals are inadequately prepared for their roles as early interventionists (weitzner-lin et al., 1994; delport, 1998). the nurses' responses to questions relating to screening are particularly concerning. the fact that a limited number of the nurses were aware of the pivotal role of communication for the early identification of at-risk children is significant. this finding implies that the nurses may not be paying sufficient attention to the communication domain during developmental screenings. research findings by rossetti (1996) clearly emphasize that despite the interdependence among the cognitive, communicative, social and motor domains of development; communication remains an extremely sensitive indicator of an infant's overall development. the nurses' positive responses to this question after the training programme illustrate the positive role that training can play in enhancing their knowledge. the fact that the majority of the nurses displayed limited knowledge about risk factors that contribute to developmental pathology and specifically communication delay, is also significant. the nurses' inadequate knowledge regarding prematurity and low birth weight is especially important as the literature identifies the two as the most important high risk factors that contribute to developmental delay in general, and communication delay in particular (rossetti, 1996). similar findings regarding the nurses' limited awareness of the potential negative effects of low birth weight and feeding problems on communication table 7 community nurses' knowledge regarding the significance of the communication domain, as reflected in the preand post-training surveys: (question dl) significance of the communication domain delayed cognitive development delayed motor development delayed communication development delayed psycho-social development a. the most common symptom of developmental disability in children under 3 years of age is ... questionnaire 1 25% 54.2% 20.8% 0% questionnaire 2 0% 0% 95.8% 4.2% questionnaire 3 9.5% 4.8% 85.7 % 0% cognitive skill motor skill communication skill psycho-social skill b. the skill that has the highest predictive correlation with a child's later intelligence attainment and school performance is ... questionnaire 1 i i 58.3% 0% 37.5 % 4.2% questionnaire 2 j· " 4.2% 0% 91.7% 4.2% questionnaire 3 4.8% 0% 95.2% 0% cognitive skill motor skill communication skill psycho-social skill c. the developmental domain that consistently separates low-risk from high-risk children is the ... questionnaire 1 25% . 4.2% 33.3% 37.5% questionnaire 2 8.3% 0% 87.5% 4.2% questionnaire 3 1 9.5% 0% 81% 9.5% die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 legini moodley, brenda louw and hugo development in infants, were found in the study by louw and weber (1997). it appears that generally, south african community nurses' knowledge with respect to the latter areas appears to be inadequate. the nurses' inadequate pre-training knowledge regarding most of the risk factors that were specific to south africa's third world, multicultural and multilingual context is also noteworthy. the nurses' generally negative responses implied that they were not supportive of the transactional model of infant development that is currently advocated in the literature (rossetti, 1996). the positive effects of training are again evident in the nurses' post-training responses. the training programme did not significantly influence the community nurses' specialized knowledge regarding the identification of infants and toddlers at-risk for communication disorders. the community nurses appeared to be well versed in identifying communication symptoms in at-risk children. this finding was also evident in the study by louw and weber (1997) and may consequently imply that information regarding the symptoms of communication disorders is currently provided by the nurses' discipline-specific training. despite the anova results indicating that the nurses' specialized knowledge for identification of at-risk children appeared satisfactory, the fact that approximately 30% of the sample were not aware of the potential adverse effects of factors that could affect the reliability of assessment results, is significant. according to iglesias and quinn (1997), social and environmental factors could influence the reliability of assessments administered in infancy and childhood. in south africa, the possibility of incorrect identification is great, as the nurses indicated during the training programme that one of the main problems experienced was that children were brought to the clinic by people who were not the primary caregiver. in addition, they stated that cultural and language differences between the children/caregivers and themselves hampered their implementation of the developmental screening assessments. approximately 67% of the sample of nurses in this study were english first language speakers who did not share the language and culture of the majority black population of south africa (pickering et al., 1998). lynch and hanson (1993) as well as iglesias and quinn (1997) assert that the ethnic, culture, and language discrepancies between service providers and families can have a significant effect on the delivery of effective eci services. the community nurses' inadequate specialized knowledge regarding the referral and monitoring of at-risk children serves to further support the need for relevant educational and training programmes. although the nurses appeared to be knowledgeable about the role of the speechlanguage therapist and audiologist, many of them were not aware of the role played by the speech-language therapist and audiologist in the management of children's feeding problems. once again, these findings concur with those of louw and weber (1997). the nurses' generally limited levels of specialized knowledge regarding referral and monitoring is significant since many of them revealed prior to and after training, that referrals would also be made to other health professionals as well as the speech-language therapist and audiologist. a possible explanation for this finding could relate to the limited table 8 community nurses' knowledge of risk factors for communication disorders : (question d4) risk factors % yes % uncertain % no risk factors ql q2 q3 ql q2 q3 ql q2 q3 1. prematurity 66.7 91.7 90.5 8.3 0 0 25 8.3 9.5 2. low birth weight 41.7 87.5 85.7 12.5 0 0 45.8 12.5 14.3 3. developmental motor delay 58.3 91.7 90.5 12.5 0 0 29.2 8.3 9.5 4. syndromes 100 95.8 100 0 0 0 0 4.2 0 5. anatomical defects 87.5 .91.7 95.2 4.2 4.2 0 8.3 4.2 4.8 . 6. feeding problems 45.8 91.7 90.5 25 4.2 4.8 29.2 4.2 4.8 i 7. poor parent-child interaction 100 100 100 0 0 0 0 0 0 ι 8. hearing problems 100 100 100 0 0 0 0 0 0 9. frequent hospitalization 58.3 95.8 95.2 25 0 4.8 16.7 4.2 0 10. the family's poor educational status 33.3 83.3 81 12.5 0 0 54.2 16.7 19 11·: the family's poor socio-economic status 37.5 87.5 81 8.3 0 0 54.2 12.5 19 12. the family's cultural background 37.5 87.5 76.2 12.5 0 0 50 12.5 23.8 13. the family's language background 45.8 87.5 71.4 20.8 0 0 33.3 y 12.5 28.6 i key: q l = questionnaire 1 q2 = questionnaire 2 q3 = questionnaires the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: a transdisciplinary model of service delivery 37 utilization of community-based practice by speech-language therapists and audiologists in south africa (uys & hugo, 1997). this situation would also possibly explain why the nurses have a poor knowledge of speech-language therapy and audiology resources in the community. when interpreting the significant increase in the community nurses' specialized knowledge for eci, it is important to note that these findings could be attributed to the possible effects of testing, experimenter bias, experimental mortality, and the formulation of questions in the questionnaire. with respect to the nurses' attitudes, the fact that they already possessed positive attitudes and beliefs about the outcome of eci, even before training, is encouraging as it reflects the current strength of the nurses' positive attitudes toward eci. finally, despite the positive findings relating to the nurses' attitudes, their responses to some of the questions are disturbing and consequently warrant further analysis and discussion. the perception that an infant's communication development can only be assessed after the child has said the first word (question d3 in table 6), reflects that these nurses appear not to be aware of the concept of continuum of risk. according to this concept, the beginning point for delayed communication skills occurs much earlier than when the child fails to say the first word at approximately one year of age (rossetti, 1996). thus, the nurses' current incorrect perception could have a detrimental effect on the early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers. the nurses' positive response toward receiving assistance from speech-language therapists and audiologists in the form of in-service training programmes is encouraging. although their responses can be viewed as being socially appropriate (rosnow & rosenthal, 1996) similar findings by louw and weber (1997) and delport (1998), serve to confirm this result. in summary, the nurses' positive attitudes and limited specialized knowledge base for eci before training, augurs well for the development of collaborative partnerships with speech-language therapists and audiologists. community nurses' evaluation of the inservice training programme with respect to objective two, all the nurses stated that the training programme had a positive effect on their knowledge and attitudes regarding eci services. the nurses appeared to be satisfied with most areas of the content and structure utilized in this study, except for the presentation time. the majority preferred training programmes to be presented in the mornings rather than afternoons, and over one day, instead of two afternoons. the nurses' suggestions regarding suitable training methods is also noteworthy, as their preferences for the use of videos, case studies and onsite visits clearly demonstrate their need for methods that depict the real life context (bennet, watson & raab, 1991). in conclusion, the overall results of this study clearly reflect that the interdisciplinary in-service training programme did significantly influence the community nurses' knowledge but not their attitudes toward the early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers (0-3 years). conclusions and implications the main finding of this study supports the speech-language therapist and audiologist's use of interdisciplinary in-service training programmes to enhance the knowledge and attitudes of community nurses regarding eci. various implications arise with regard to the role of speech-language therapists and their education and training. however, the conclusions and implications are made cautiously in acknowledgement of the study's exploratory nature and the limitations thereof {leedy, 1993). in view of the importance of the research topic for eci in south africa, it seems reasonable to make some tentative recommendations based on the findings of this study. implications for the roles of speechlanguage therapists and audiologists in the delivery of early communication intervention services in south africa the following implications are identified: adoption of the transdisciplinary team approach to increase accessibility to eci it is recommended that speech-language therapists and audiologists adopt the transdisciplinary team approach as it facilitates the ability of early interventionists to collaborate, and thus provide community-based eci services, as supported by the national rehabilitation policy (1997). the current study clearly demonstrated that by adopting the principles of role expansion and role release, speechlanguage therapists and audiologists are able to utilize interdisciplinary training programmes to improve community nurses' knowledge regarding early identification and appropriate referral of children who are at-risk for developing communication disorders. by expanding their roles from that of service provider to that of educator, speech-language therapists and audiologists become brokers of information (rossetti, 1996). this role enables them to extend their services from the tertiary level of service delivery to that of the secondary and primary levels where at-risk children and families are located. uys and hugo (1997) and delport (1998) emphasize the dire need for speech-language therapists and audiologists to take their services to the clients, rather than waiting for clients to come to the service. collaboration: implications for recommended practice in eci within the constraints of the limited number of qualified speech-language therapists and audiologists currently practising in south africa, it is imperative that speechlanguage therapists and audiologists are proactive in determining appropriate ways of delivering eci services at family health clinics. the community-based model of intervention proposed by fair and louw (1999) may be utilised to guide further research regarding the nature of eci services that south african speech-language therapists and audiologists can provide at family health clinics in south africa. the significance of the concept of collaboration for the role of speech-language therapists and audiologists in eci is evident, however, the current study reflects that the nature of the collaborative relationship appears to differ according to the purpose and people involved in the collaborative partnership. it is clear that speech-language therapists and audiologists need to be competent in die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 legini moodley, brenda louw and hugo developing collaborative relationships with personnel at different levels of the health care system. for example, with key personnel at national level for advocacy purposes, with administrators at management level to gain entry into the discipline specific health service, with health providers in the community, and finally, with families with at-risk children. it is essential that further research is conducted on the nature of the collaborative process that is suitable for the different types of collaborative partnerships in eci. pickering et al. (1998) emphasize the need for identifying professional competencies that transcend those of specific disabilities, and that include those related to interdisciplinary team work in the delivery of community-based service delivery. marketing speech-language therapy and audiology services in addressing the nurses' poor awareness of speechlanguage therapy and audiology services in the community, it is recommended that south african speech-language therapists and audiologists need to increase their role relating to the marketing of eci services for at-risk children (louw, 1997). a six step cross-disciplinary marketing approach devised by fugate and fugate (1996) provides an effective and efficient method of generating professional and public awareness about eci services. training of speech-language therapists and audiologists the present insufficient numbers of qualified speechlanguage therapists and audiologists in south africa clearly highlight the need for the training of more personnel to cope with the current and projected demand for eci services (pickering et al, 1998). in addressing this need, however, it is crucial that recruitment efforts are directed towards increasing the cultural and linguistic diversity of students that is reflective of those shared by the majority population in the country (uys & hugo, 1997; pickering et al., 1998). implication for the education and training of south african speech-language therapists and audiologists in early communication intervention the provision of more indirect, consultative and collaborative services requires speech-language therapists and audiologists to be competent and skilled in areas that they may not have been adequately trained in, during their pre-service years (weitzner-lin et al., 1994). it is therefore important that these areas are identified and adequately addressed in education and training programmes. the need for further training in the development of collaborative partnerships and implementation of interdisciplinary training programmes during the implementation of this study, it became evident that in order for speech-language therapists and audiologists to be able to transform their roles and utilize an alternate model of intervention, they needed to be competent in two vital areas in which they traditionally received little training. these areas are, firstly; collaboration with other health care providers utilizing the principles of the transdisciplinary team model; and secondly, the planning and implementation of interdisciplinary in-service programmes for the education and training of other early intervention professionals. in the implementation of services to at-risk children and their families, it is evident that speech-language therapists and audiologists require specialized attitudes, knowledge and skills that are not adequately provided during their pre-service training (weitzner-lin et al., 1994; delport, 1998). therefore, besides reinforcing the recommendations of delport (1998) regarding the need for further training in the identified areas, it is also recommended that competencies and specific outcomes are clearly identified according to the guidelines of the national qualifications framework. this will serve to guide the development of an appropriate curriculum for undergraduate and continuing educational and training programmes. planning appropriate training programmes the findings of weitzner-lin et al. (1994) are important in the planning of appropriate training efforts, as they indicate that formal course work was not the method through which most speech-language therapists obtained their specialized knowledge base. this study recommended that workshops and in-service.programmes needed to be structured around learning experiences that provided therapists with more hands-on experience with at-risk children and their families. the guidelines provided by roberts et al., (1995) may be used in the development of programmes for speech-language therapists and audiologists' specialization in early intervention. in conclusion, the results of the present exploratory study clearly demonstrate the suitability and potential of collaborative team initiatives in expanding the delivery of eci services in contexts that are plagued with limited financial and human resources. what is evident is that collaborative enterprises present early interventionists, like speech-language therapists and audiologists and community nurses, with challenges that highlight the need for transformation in their respective roles and responsibilities toward the delivery of efficacious eci services to the atrisk population. future research efforts are required to assist early interventionists in this regard by ensuring that solutions proposed are theoretically grounded and practically feasible within the developing south african context. | ι acknowledgements j this article is based on a thesis submitted to the university of pretoria for the masters degree , in communication pathology. assistance is acknowledged from professors brenda louw and rene hugo, as well as colleagues at the university of durban-westville, harsha karthard and cyril govender. references a national health plan for south africa. (april, 1994). prepared by the anc with the technical support of who and unicef. american association of colleges of nursing. (1996). interdisciplinary education and practice. journal of professional nursing, 12, (2), 119-123. ' american speech-language hearing 'association. (1989). communication-based services for infants, toddlers, and their families. asha, may, 32-34. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) early identification of at-risk infants and toddlers: a transdisciplinary model of service delivery 39 american speech-language-hearing association (1991). the prevention of communication disorders: tutorial. asha, 33 (suppl.6), 15-41. bennet, t., watson, a.l. & raab, m. (1995). ensuring competence in early intervention personnel through personnel standards and high-quality training. in blackman, j.a. (ed.). training and continuing education in early intervention (ppl96-208). united states of america: aspen publishers, inc. brandt, p.a. & magyary, d.l. (1989). preparation of clinical nurse specialists for family centred early intervention. infants and young children, 1,(3), 51-62. briggs, m.h. (1993). team talk: communication skills for early intervention teams. journal of childhood communication disorders, 15, (1), 33-40. briggs, m.h. (1997). building early intervention teams. working together for children and families. gaithersburg: aspen publishers, inc. brink, h.i. (1996). fundamentals of research methodology for health care professionals. cape town: juta & co, ltd. cox, a.w. (1996). preparing nurses. in bricker, d. & widerstrom, a. (eds.). preparing personnel to work with infants and young children and their families. a team approach (pp.161-179). baltimore: paul brookes publishing co. delport, j. (1998). 'n diensleweringsmodel van vroee kommunikasie intervensie op die suid-afrikaanse platteland: opleiding van verpleegkundiges oor die identifikasie van jong kinders (0-3 jaar) met 'n hoerisiko vir kommunikasieprobleme. unpublished masters thesis, university of pretoria, south africa. department of health. (november, 1995). towards a national health system. draft for discussion from the deputy directorgeneral: policy and planning. pretoria. fair, l. & louw, b. (1999). early communication intervention within a community-based intervention model in south africa. the south african journal of communication disorders, 46, 13-23. fugate, d.l. & fugate, j.m. (1996). putting the marketing plan to work: practical suggestions for early intervention programs. infants and young children, 8, (4), 70-79. hanson, m.j. & brekken, l.j. (1991). early intervention personnel model and standards: an interdisciplinary fielddeveloped approach. infants and young children, 4, (1), 5461. hugo, s.r. (1996). open letter to all participants in the curriculum development programme. national inter-university curriculum meeting. university of pretoria. south africa. iglesias, a. & quinn, r. (1997). culture as a context for early intervention. in thurman, s.k., cornwell, j.r. & gottwald, s.r. (eds.). contexts of early intervention (pp 55-71). baltimore: paul brookes publishing company. leedy, p.d. (1993). practical research: planning and design. new / york: macmillan publishing company. louw, b. (1997). early communication intervention: guidelines for speech-language therapists and audiologists. sashla: ethics and standards committee. louw, b. & kritzinger, a. (1991). infants with down's syndrome: description of an early intervention approach. the south african journal of communication disorders, 38, 25-31. louw, b. & weber, i. (1997). attitudes and perceptions of nurses regarding early communication intervention. health sa gesondheid, 2, (2), 16-22. lynch, e.w. & hanson, m.j. (1993). changing demographics: implications for training in early intervention. in blackman, j.a. (1995). (ed.). training and continuing education in early intervention (pp. 209 217). united states of america: aspen publishers, inc. moodley, l. (1999). an in-service training programme for community nurses in the identification of at-risk infants and toddlers. unpublished masters thesis, university of pretoria, south africa. | national health plan for south africa. (april, 1994). prepared by the anc with the technical support of who and unicef. national rehabilitation policy. (december, 1997). rehabilitation for all. second draft. department of health. south africa. pickering, m., mcallister, l., hagler, p., whitehill, t.l., penn, c., robertson, s.j. & mccready, v. (1998). external factors influencing the profession in six societies. american journal of speech-language pathology, 7, (4), 5-17. roberts, j.e., crais, e., layton, t., watson, l. & reinhartsen, d. (1995). developing an early intervention preservice specialization in speech-language pathology. american journal of speech language pathology, 4, (2), 31-36. rosnow, r.l. & rosenthal, r. (1996). beginning behavioural research. a conceptual primer. new jersey: prentice-hall, inc. rossetti, l.m. (1993). enhancing early intervention services to infants/toddlers and their families. journal of childhood communication disorders, 15, (1), 1-6. rossetti, l.m. (1996). communication intervention. birth to three. san diego: singular publishing group, inc. trivette, c.m., dunst, c.j., hamby, d.w. & lapointe, m.a. (1996). key elements of empowerment and their implications for early intervention. infant-toddler intervention. the transdisciplinary journal, 6, (1), 59-73. uys, i. & hugo, r. (1997). speech-language pathology and audiology: transformation in teaching, research and service delivery. health sa gesondheid, 2, (2), 23-29. weitzner-lin, b., chambers, d., & sierpierski, j. (1994). what specialized knowledge is needed to provide early intervention services in children's hospitals? infant-toddler intervention. the transdisciplinary journal, 4, (2), 87-104. white paper on integrated national disability strategy. (november, 1997). office of the deputy president, t.m. mbeki. rustica press. winton, (1996). a model for supporting higher education faculty in their early intervention personnel preparation roles. infants and young children, 8, (3), 56-67. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 47, 2000 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) fwift low noise, low distortion sound easy to use quick and easy programming high reliability cellphone compatible cost effective vf \ \ , \ ergo! excellent sound quality attractive design high reliability simple programming new advanced features cellphone compatible cost effective then hearing is believing. if you're not yet convinced that oticon leads the way in programmable hearing instruments, we invite you to try the ergo, swiftordigilife.com range for yourself. ergo redefines the fundamentals of hearing care while swift and digilife.com introduce a new concept in affordable hearing care. digilifejcom /digifocusjt advanced digital technology virtually invisible interactive website benefits quick, easy hearing care options cost effective • λ c a l p h o n t c o oticon sounds good to you toll free 0800 650 750 or visit us at www.oticon.com and digilife.com ο i ro r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) http://www.oticon.com a comparative study of inferential comprehension strategies between a language-learing disabled and non language-learning disabled child yael tombak, ba (speech and hearing therapy) (witwatersrand) glenda shapiro, ba (log) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg abstract the aim of this study was to analyse and compare the inferential comprehension strategies used by a language-learning disabled and non language-learning disabled subject, as well as to investigate their performance on areas related to inferential comprehension. for these purposes, tests were either constructed or modified in order to allow for qualitative analysis of the subjects' responses and the strategies used. the non language-learning disabled subject was found to utilize efficient inferential strategies, suggestive of cognitive-linguistic integrity, whereas the language-learning disabled subject was found to use inefficient inferential strategies and to be deficient on several areas related to inferential comprehension. these findings are interpreted as being reflective of a breakdown in the interactional dynamics between cognition and language. opsomming die doel van hierdie studie was om die afleidende-begripsstrategiee wat deur 'n taalleergestremde en 'n nie-taalleergestremde proefpersoon gebruik word, te ontleed en te vergelyk. hulle prestasie in aspekte wat aan afleidende begrip verwant is, is ook ondersoek. vir hierdie doel is toetse opgestel of aangepas om kwalitatiewe ontleding van die proefpersone se response en die strategiee wat hulle gebruik, uit te voer. die nie-taalleergestremde kind het doeltreffende afleidende strategiee gebruik wat kognitiewe linguistiese integriteit aandui. die taalleergestremde kind het ontoereikende strategiee gebruik en was ook ondoeltreffend in aspekte wat aan afleidende begrip verwant is. die bevindings word interpreteer as bewys van 'n disintegrasie van die interafhanklike dinamiek tussen kognitiewe en linguistiese funksie. inferential comprehension refers to the listener's ability to use his real world knowledge, in combination with linguistic information to go beyond the explicitly stated information. thus, a person's ability to engage in inferential comprehension depends on active participation, as well as utilization of a wide variety of stored information, pragmatics and author-reader conventions (thorndyke, 1976). inferential ability is an important component of comprehension and communication. furthermore, it is an important prerequisite for dealing with information in many academic tasks (klein-konigsberg, 1984). the principles of inferential ability are important in reading comprehension (kail et al. 1977). furthermore, language demands placed on students in the academic setting require them "to understand and follow the teacher's directions and to focus and derive main ideas from the teacher's lecture, to organize and store these facts for retrieval" (o'connor and eldredge, cited by nelson, 1986). 1 both cognitive and linguistic hypotheses have been proposed to explain inferential disability (crais and chapman, 1987; ellis weismer, 1985) but only recently has inferential comprehension become an area of interest in the field of language pathology and learning disabilities (freston and drew, cited by crais and chapman, 1987; ellis weismer, 1985; klein-konigsberg, 1984). until recently, most studies of language-disabled children have focused primarily on syntax and morphology (wiig and semel, 1981). the recent shift toward the study of this area — previously the domain of psychological research (bransford and franks, 1971; johnson, bransford and solomon, 1973; paris and carter, 1973; paris and lindauer, 1976) — holds significant theoretical and practical implications relevant to the field of language problems. these implications could have an impact on our current conceptualization of cognitive and linguistic processes that operate in various language disorders, and on the ensuing course of intervention. previous research on inferential ability has revealed some interesting trends. for example, it was noted that younger children lacked a deliberate strategic approach when engaging in inferential comprehension tasks (paris and lindauer, 1976). younger children experienced difficulty integrating premises and manipulating linguistic information in memory, and their schemata were fragmented (danner & mathews, 1980). older children were found to employ more deliberate metamemorial strategies to integrate premises (paris & lindauer, 1976). their verbal representational skills were superior and their schemata richer and more comprehensive (danner & mathews, 1980). in the light of this developmental perspective it was hypothesized that perhaps language-learning disabled children would use earlier developing strategies which are less goal-orientated and task-appropriate. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 © sasha 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 there has been little research into deficient inferential abilities. as with the research on the normal inferential process, these studies were flawed by the lack of an adequate conceptual model or systematic delineation of the abilities examined. despite the scarcity of research on inferential disability in the language-learning disabled population, some trends have emerged from the literature. for example, snyder (cited by ellis weismer, 1985) noted that languagelearning disabled children made fewer inferences as do younger children, and also that their performance is characterized by difficulties with simultaneous analysis and synthesis of information. some writers (carroll, 1986), have suggested that the language-learning disabled children have poorly integrated schemata, whilst crais and chapman (1987) attributed inferential disability to poor verbal comprehension. recent literature on the nature of learning disabilities reflects an orientation which considers strategic inefficiency to be central to academic under-achievement (reed & hresko; torgeson; cited by wiig and secord, 1985). therefore, the primary aim of this study (tombak, 1987) was to analyse and compare the inferential comprehension strategies used by a language-learning disabled and non languagelearning disabled child. a strategy-based approach was used as it was considered to be a valid method for qualitative investigation of linguistic cognitive processes. with this in mind, the writer (tombak, 1987) conceptualised a model of inferential comprehension in which the inferential comprehension process may be viewed as an interaction between the text and what the listener brings with him to the comprehension process, resulting in the product or inference (see figure 1). yael tombak and glenda shapiro the contextual basis of inferential ability was considered in terms of the 'microstructure' (carroll, 1986), and the linguistic factors which may affect comprehension. listener variables were considered in terms of macrostructure and schematic knowledge. these refer to the listener's knowledge of the standard arrangement of information and generalized experiential knowledge respectively (moates and schumacher, 1980). problem-solving strategies were also taken into account. lastly, the final inference product was analysed in terms of inferential subject matter and the role of an interceding inference. this model attempted to delineate the main factors involved in the inferential comprehension process. it also aimed to provide a method of systematic task analysis, in order to establish and interpretive basis for the subjects' inferential performance and the strategies used. aims the primary aim of this study was to analyse and compare the inferential comprehension strategies used by a languagelearning disabled and non language-learning disabled child. the specific aims of the study were to describe the different inferential strategies used by these two children in a systematic, qualitative manner and to investigate the influence of selected task factors on the strategies used. subject description si was a 13.0 year old learning-disabled male. he was a standard 5 pupil at a remedial school which he had been attending for the past 2 years. on the wechsler intelligence figure 1: model of inferential comprehension 2. 3. microstructure • text organization • contextual basis text lexical implications intersentential relations 2. linguistic factors [—vocabulary i— embedding i— passivization subject matter .— event • subjective state inference 2. interceding inference macrostructure schemata/script strategies listener i the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparative study of inferential comprehension strategies 5 scale for children — revised, he obtained a full sclae intelligence quotient of 101, indicating intellectual functioning within the normal range. there was no significant discrepancy between his verbal and performance intelligence quotients. si experienced difficulty in the areas of reading and spelling, scoring 2-4 years below his chronological age level on formal reading and spelling tests. narrative language sampling and formal testing of auditory memory for paragraphs and story material (see table 1, criterion tests), revealed age-appropriate performance. language comprehension was not considered a subject selection criterion because this component was evaluated as a\correlation factor to inferential ability. s2 was a 12.9 year old non learning-disabled male in standard 5 at a regular school. there was no history of academic difficulty and school progress was good. narrative language sampling and formal testing of auditory memory for paragraphs and story material (see table 1, criterion tests), revealed age-appropriate performance. description of test battery (see table 1) table 1: the test battery criterion tests 1. c.e.l.f.:— subtest 6 (semel & wiig, 1980) 2. story recall "the stork in the wheat" (berry, 1969) 3. narrative tasks:— sequence story, poster picture and perso• nal narratives (westby, 1984) the test battery administered was considered under the following three categories:j— a) criterion tests: these were. tests utilized to assess the subject's fulfilment of preselection criteria. b) correlation tests: these tasks were used to evaluate the subjects' performance in areas related to inferential comprehension.. c) main test: these constituted the primary tests of inferential comprehension. a) criterion tests tests of auditory memory clinical evaluation of language functions (c.e.l.f., semel and wiig, 1980) subtest 6 — processing spoken paragraphs. story recall (berry, 1969) a test story, "the stork in the wheat", was used to measure retention and recall of salient sequential information in a story. expressive language tasks (westby, 1984) expressive language was evaluated using three narrative language tasks: a sequence story narrative, poster picture narrative and personal narrative. responses to the stimuli were evaluated in terms of 'spontaneous inferencing', or spontaneously generated inferential elaboration. b) correlation tests adequate grammatic comprehension, receptive vocabulary and schematic knowledge are considered to be critical for linguistic comprehension. these areas were tested to examine the relationship between these factors and inferential ability. grammatic comprehension tests test of adolescent language (t.o.a.l.; hammill et al. 1980) subtest 2 — listening/grammar embedding test this test was adapted from a test constructed by penn (1972) and was designed to assess comprehension of relative, complement and multiple-embedded sentences. main tests 1. spontaneous inferencing 2. t.o.l.c:— bubtest 2 (wiig & secord, 1985) 3. inferential comprehension test 1 4. inferential comprehension test 2:— microstructure, embedding, passivization, vocabularly, interceding, inference wiig-semel test of linguistic concepts — passive concept (wiig & semel, 1976) receptive vocabulary peabody picture vocabulary test — form a (p.p.v.t.; dunn, 1965). schematic knowledge schema test this test, adapted from bower, black and turner (cited by reed, 1982), was designed to assess the comprehensiveness of the subjects' schemata for five common situations involving event and action sequences. c) main tests evaluation of spontaneous inferencing the sequence story and poster narratives were analysed in correlation tests 1. t.o.a.l.:— subtest 2 (hammill et al. 1980) 2. embedding test (adapted from penn, 1972) 3. wiig-semel test passive concept (wiig & semel, 1976) 4. p . p . v . t . : f o r m a (dunn, 1965) 5. schema test (adapted from bower, black & turner; cited by reed, 1982) die suid-afkaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 5, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 yael tombak and glenda shapiro terms of the spontaneous inferences generated. test of language competence (t.o.l.c.; wiig and secord, 1985) subtest 2 making inferences (see appendix 1) this subtest evaluated the ability to make causal inferences based on existing event chains in which one or more causal links were missing. where appropriate, the subjects were probed for linguistic explanation, justification and elaboration of their responses. inferential comprehension test 1 (see appendix 2) this was the first of two tasks devised by the experimenter, specifically for this study, in order to evaluate inferential strategies. items consisted of sentence pairs followed by a question, and were organized according to the conceptual model outlined earlier (see figure 1). the inferential abilities required for these tasks were primarily based on real world knowlege. the tasks were linguistically simple to minimize the influence of linguistic factors. inferential comprehension test 2 (see appendix 3) this test used a paragraph format and was more reliant on linguistic factors for comprehension. each of the factors considered was represented by a pair of thematically similiar paragraphs, one of which was systematically varied with regard to that factor. each paragraph was followed by questions and probing. methods of analysis criterion and correlation tests these were scored according to test manual instructions, or specially devised scoring systems for each test, which were constructed for the purpose of the study. main tests evaluation of spontaneous inferencing the number of spontaneous inferences generated by the subjects was calculated and the nature of the inferences qualitatively described. test'of language competence — subtest 2 (wiig and secord; 1985) i ι i four analyses were carried out on the response data obtained:^ / quantitative analysis — scoring based on point calculation was utilised. quantitative analysis of strategies — the t.o.l.c. responses were analised qualitatively by the examiner. the responses were organized by the examiner into 12 major categories of deficient strategies (see results section). divergent shift analysis — this analysis considered the subjects' ability to make divergent conceptual shifts. congruency analysis — this analysis considered the subjects' attention to semantic congruency between response alternatives. test of inferential comprehension 1 the responses were qualitatively analysed in terms of strategies. test of inferential comprehension 2 the paragraph pairs were analysed in terms of strategy patterns, divergence, and the effect of factor variation on performance. results and discussion the subjects' performance on the criterion, correlation and main tests were analyzed quantitatively and qualitatively, and compared. their responses on the main tests were considered to reflect the inferential strategies utilized. a) criterion tests both subjects scored above the revised pass-fail criterion of 14 on the c.e.l.f. subtest 6 (semel & wiig, 1980) indicating the presence of adequate auditory memory for paragraph material. on the story memory task (berry, 1969), both subjects scored above 70%, calculated using a scoring system devised by the examiner (tombak, 1987). the scores were interpreted as reflective of adequate auditory memory for complex'story material. both subjects fulfilled the criterion of adequate verbal expression on the narrative task battery. however, although both subjects were able to verbally express their ideas in a clear manner, their expressive abilities differed along the dimensions of spontaneous inferencing and coherence in favour of s2. b) correlation tests (see table 2) si's performance was below average on the following tasks: ι — t.o.a.l. subtest 2 (hammill et al. 1980) i — embedding test i — test of linguistic concepts — passive concept (wiig & semel, 1976) | , — description of schematic events, which was fragmented. si performed adequately on the p.p.v.t. — form a (dunn, 1965) s2's performance was average to above-average on all the above tasks. of significance was his much superior performance on the p.p.v.t, — form a (dunn, 1 9 6 5 ) / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comparative study of inferential comprehension strategies 7 t a b l e 2: correlation tests si s2 t.o.a.l. below average scaled score = 5 average scaled score = 10 embedding test poor percentage = 43% good percentage = 87% wiig — semel test std 1 level percentage = 70% std 6 level percentage = 100% p.p.v.t. average mental age = 12.7 yrs percentile = 45 above average mental age = 18 + yrs percentile = 94 schema test poor raw score = 27 good raw score = 41 c) main tests evaluation of spontaneous inferencing there was a marked difference between the subjects' spontaneous inferencing abilities, both quantitatively and qualitatively. spontaneous inferencing was measured quantitatively in units, each unit being an inferential proposition or item of information. this scoring system was developed for the purpose of the study (tombak, 1987). unit scoring was carried out by the examiner. si generated a total of 4 units (picture narrative — 2 units, poster narrative — 2 units), in contrast to a total of 30 units produced by s2 (picture narrative — 12 units; poster narrative — 18 units). si's narratives lacked elaboration, and were concrete and highly stimulus-bound, and there was inadequate thematic content. s2's inferences were complex narrative sequences elaborating on preceding and consequent events, motivations and internal states. test of language competence quantitative analysis:— si experienced great difficulty with inferential reasoning, and selected a low proportion (43%) of correct inferential responses, while s2 selected a high proportion (87%) of correct responses. qualitative analysis:— this analysis was devised to compare and contrast the nature of the deficient strategies used by the 2 subjects. as stated earlier, the responses were organized by the examiner into 12 categories of deficient strategies (see figure 2). the first ten of these were grouped into four strategy clusters, designated by the letters (a) — (d). each cluster corresponded to a number of ineffective ways of implementing a constructive strategy. strategy cluster a: divergent thinking ι figure 2: strategy analysis 1.1 concretism 1.2 conceptual syncretism 2. lexical syncretism 3. psychological explanation 4. linguistically-contradicted divergence 5. linguistically-unsupported divergence 6. literal comprehension difficulty 7. illogical inference i.t.o. real world knowledge 8. non-optimal inference i.t.o. real world knowledge strategy cluster b: convergent thinking strategy cluster c: utilization of real world knowledge strategy cluster d: causal reasoning die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 yael tombak and glenda shapiro a. strategy cluster a this cluster was related to difficulty with divergent thinking. 1.1 concretism concretism refers to the inability to go beyond the linguistic context presented. a concrete answer lacks hypotheticodeductive reasoning. eg. t.o.l.c item 5 (si) (e = examiner; s = subject). e: they talked to a policeman because ... s: because bob had bad luck. this category occurred frequently in si's responses as opposed to s2's, where no instances were noted. si relied heavily on linguistic context, reflecting deficient hypothetical reasoning. in contrast, s2's responses were hypotheticodeductive. 1.2 conceptual syncretism conceptual syncretism is a piagetian concept which refers to a tendency found in preoperational and concrete operational thought — to juxtapose rather than synthesise logical and causal relations (piaget, 1927). a syncretic response is characterised by an indiscriminate linkage of two verbal statements with no consideration of their conceptual relationship. eg. t.o.l.c trial item (si) e: they had to go to eat at a restaurant because... s: they had a turkey at home and they wanted to eat at a restaurant. this strategy occurred exclusively in si's responses. due to si's inability to go beyond th€ linguistic context into the realm of hypotheses, he adopted the strategy of randomly juxtaposing any two salient concepts presented simultaneously, and forming a syncretic connection. s2's reasoning, in contrast, was highly divergent and hypothetical. si's deficit was characteristic of preoperational difficulty with constructing relationships that reflect attention to and simultaneous retention of all critical relevant information. previous investigations have also documented difficulties in simultaneous analysis and synthesis of information in learning-disabled children (crais and chapman, 1987; gerber, 1981; klein-konigsberg, 1984). 2. lexical syncretism this term refers to the tendency to juxtapose any two lexical items indiscriminately, with no apparent conceptual basis for this fusion. eg. t.o.l.c. trial item (si) e: do you have any idea why ... they weren 't able to eat at home? s: because the house was trimming this tendency was a unique feature observed in si's responses, and appeared to be a primitive, and possibly deviant manifestation of conceptual syncretism, which resulted in distortion of the relationships depicted. si resorted to this preoperational strategy when he experienced a comprehension breakdown. a similar finding was also reported by klein-konigsberg (1984) who found that language-learning disabled children tended to attend to smaller sentence constituents when applying integrational strategies. 3. psychological explanation this refers to the preoperational tendency to provide a psychological explanation when a casual event explanation would be more appropriate (piaget, 1927). eg. t.o.l.c. item 1 (si) e: jack didn't leave a tip because ... s: he didn't-feel like leaving a tip. si tended to give psychological explanations of events, while s2 formulated hypothetical causal events. cluster a (1, 2, 3) represented deficits in divergent thinking. si's responses were overreliant on linguistic contest, and reflected analytic deficits. si lacked the ability to depart from reality. the strategies used by si interfered with divergence and the ability to formulate causal event relationships. snyder (cited by ellis weismer, 1985) also reported divergence deficits in a group of language-disordered children studied. b. strategy cluster β this cluster represented problems with converging to the linguistic context. 4. linguistically-contradicted divergence 7 ι this feature is associated with the tendency to ignore or misinterpret critical linguistic sequences, due to insufficient cognizance of linguistic information that suggests a contrary direction or focus of inferential convergence. eg. t.o.l.c. trial item (si) i e: they had to eat at a restaurant because ... s: they had a turkey at home and they wanted to eat at a restaurant. si used this strategy frequently, often ignoring or misinterpreting salient linguistic information. this feature also occurred in s2's responses to a lesser extent. it appeared that si's preoperational difficulty with simultaneous analysis and synthesis of information, resulted in unpermissible divergent responses. / eg. t.o.l.c. item 6 (si) e: eric was grateful to his uncle fred because ... s: (selects alternative a:) uncle fred bought himself a moped ... because his uncle didn't have a bike and then he bought one. this strategy occurred frequently in si's responses, in those the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) comparative study of inferential comprehension strategies i n s t a n c e s when he did attempt to diverge beyond the linuistic context but it did not occur in s2's responses. this strategy was used when the linguistic context was not fully u n d e r s t o o d , due to fragmented lexical schemata. poor assimilation and accommodation strategies may account for such deficient schemata. 6. literal comprehension difficulty this refers to difficulty in comprehending the basic relationships depicted, due to vocabulary deficits. eg. t.o.l.c. trial item (si) e: do you have any idea why ... they weren't able to eat at home? s: because the house was trimming. literal comprehension deficits were rare in si's responses and absent from s2's responses on this test. strategy cluster β (4, 5, 6) represented deficits in linguistic convergence. si failed to perceive several lexical implications and their interactions simultaneously, reflecting a preoperational tendency to attend to only one feature or property at a time (wiig & semel, 1984). s2, on the other hand was able to attend to the full scope of the linguistic information presented, resulting in "lawful divergence" (phillips, 1971). difficulties in simultaneous analysis and synthesis have been noted in language-learning disabled children (crais and chapman, 1987; ellis weismer, 1985; kleinkonigsberg, 1984) as well as younger normal children (paris and lindauer, 1976). c. strategy cluster c this cluster was related to ineffective utilization of real world knowledge. 7. ,,·illogical inference in terms of real world knowledge this feature refers to the formulation of inferences which are implausible in terms of real world knowledge. eg. t.o.l.c. item 5 (si) ! e: they talked to a policeman because ... s: ... because they rode on a crowded bus. only si formulated inferences that were implausible in terms of real world knowledge. it seemed that si had not acquired comprehensive schemata, as indicated by his performance on the schema test. his schemata appeared to lack refinement, and resisted absorption of new information. this deficit could also be related to inadequate accessing as well as induction of schemata (carroll, 1986). 8. non-optimal inference in terms of real world knowledge this feature is characterised by the formulation of plausible, but unlikely inferences due to inefficient utilization of real world knowledge. eg. t.o.l.c. item 1 (si) 9 e: jack didn't leave a tip because ... s: he didn't feel like it ... he was unkind. si tended to make illogical inferences, in contrast to s2 whose inferences were always plausible, although not always probable. strategy cluster c (7, 8) was concerned with the utilization of schematic knowledge in generating logical inferences. fragmented schemata result in implausible or non-optimal inferences. this strategic failure could be caused by poorly induced schemata or failure to access the appropriate schemata timeously. d. strategy cluster d this strategy was related to causal reasoning. 9. inversed cause-effect reasoning this feature refers to reasoning which is characterised by inadequate distinction between and sequencing of cause and effect events. inversed causal reasoning is characteristic of preoperational and concrete operational thinking and is associated with egocentrism and a tendency towards syncretic perception (phillips, 1971). eg. t.o.l.c. item 5 (si) e: they talked to a policeman because ... s: (selects d:) bob lost his money sometime before they got to the mall... because they couldn't pay for the bus. the expression of the causal relationship was inversed by si and an effect was formulated instead of a cause. s i ' s responses were characterised by deficient causal reasoning as his concept of 'because' was associated with a sequential meaning, rather than a true concept of causality. s2's reasoning was characterised by explicit delineation of causeeffect relationships and he was able to distinguish clearly between cause and effect. 10. egocentric reasoning egocentric explanations are characterised by presupposition of the listener's knowledge of the speaker's internal reasoning process. egocentric reasoning is a characteristic of preoperational thought. eg. t.o.l.c. item 1 (si) e: he didn't leave a tip because ... s: (selects a) the restaurant closed when he arrived. e: why do you think that's a good answer? s: because he didn't leave the waiter a tip. e: and c), 'the food and service were excellent? s: yes, the food and service were excellent. egocentric reasoning was a common feature of si's reasoning while only one instance was observed in s2's responses. egocentric explanations are characteristic of preoperational reasoning, and are caused by a lack of concern for the listener's perspective (piaget, 1923). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 yael tombak and glenda s h a p i r o strategy cluster d (9, 10) was concerned with the role of causal reasoning in inferencing. si's reasoning was characterised by poor comprehension of cause-effect relationships and egocentrism, reflective of a cognitive developmental lag (phillips, 1971). s2 was able to engage in formal-operational explicit causal reasoning. 11. interceding inference when a final inference relies on an intermediate step or inference, the task is complicated since a faulty interceding inference may lead to the induction of a faulty final inference. eg. t.o.l.c. item 9 (si) lori took the bus downtown because it was her mother's birthday. she left the fashionable stores with tears in her eyes. e: lori cried because ... s: maybe her mother didn't have any money with her so she could buy clothes and that ... the interceding inference that 'lori went to town to buy her mother a birthday present' was not included. si's inferential reasoning frequently broke down as a result of faulty interceding inferences. s2 was generally able to generate correct interceding inferences. 12. alternative inference this category was related to difficulty with divergent conceptual shifting. eg. t.o.l.c. item 6 (si) e: eric was grateful to his uncle fred because ... s: because his uncle let him use the bike for a long time. e: can you think of another reason why he was grateful? s: because his uncle let him ride it. si consistently offered a paraphrase of previous explanations as an alternative inference, retaining the same conceptual basis for all interpretations (wiig & secord, 1985). s2's responses were characterised by alternative interpretations and a more divergent orientation. si's responses reflected deficits in divergent conceptual shifting, resulting in overreliance on linguistic context. this finding was in agreement with that of snyder (cited by ellis weisman, 1985) who found that language-disordered children made fewer inferences than age peers. difficulty with conceptual shifting has also been documented in the learning disabled population (wiig & semel, 1976). the difference in divergence abilities appeared to be diagnostically significant. this deficit appeared to be central to si's inferential disability. divergent shift analysis sl'.s responses were characterised by few conceptual shifts (total = 3 conceptual shifts) and a low proportion (30%) of matching inferences. s2's responses were divergent, with a high level of conceptual shifting (total = 10 conceptual shifts) and a high proportion of matching inferences (90%). eg. poor conceptual shifting item 6 (subject 1) e: eric was grateful to his uncle fred because ... s: (1) because his uncle could let him use the bike ... (2) because his uncle let him ride it. number of matching inferences in above example = 0/2. neither of the above inferences matched the correct responses listed for item 6 (b, d). eg. good conceptual shifting item 1 (subject 2) e: jack didn't leave a tip because — s: (1) because the restaurant wasn't good. the food was bad and the service was bad. " " (2) he never had any small change on him. number of matching inferences in above example = 2/2. these 2 inferences above matched the correct responses listed for item 1 (b, d). congruency analysis si tended to ignore congruency between response alternatives, reflecting difficulty with simultaneous analysis and synthesis, suggesting a cognitive lag. this difficulty has been noted in the language-learning disabled population (crais and chapman, 1987; ellis weismer, 1985), as well as in younger normal children (paris and lindauer, 1976). test of inferential comprehension 1 both subjects performed adequately on this test, which relied primarily on schematic knowledge, rather than linguistic context. however, s2's responses were more divergent, and contained more conceptual shifting. test of inferential comprehension 2 this test was analysed in terms of the effect of factor variation on inferential ability (see table 3). it was found that the factors or forced reliance on schematic knowledge, reversible embedding and passivization, and unclear intersentential relationships, had an adverse effect on only si's performance. both subjects' performance was adversely affected by complex vocabulary level and the presence of an interceding inference. however, s i ' s problems resulted in more extensive utilization of the deficient strategies described in the t.o.l.c. analysis. table 3: results of inferential comprehension test 2 j factors si s2 ! vocabulary ( ) ( ) schematic knowledge ( ) n.e. embedding (nr) n.e. n.e. (r> ( ) n.e. passivization (nr) n.e. n.e. (r) ( ) n.e. microstructure ( ) / n.e. interceding inference ( ) · ' ( ) key: nr = non-reversible r = reversible n.e. = no effect on performance ( ) = adverse effect on performance the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparative study of inferential comprehension strategies 11 non-reversible passivization and embedding had no effect on the subjects' comprehension, since the paragraph content was highly predictable due to the presence of semantic constraints. s i ' s breakdown in inferential reasoning, precipitated by some of the factors listed above, paralleled his inferior results on the correlation tests. this confirmed previous findings of such deficits in the learning-disabled population (wiig & semel, 1976; 1984). s2's performance was not affected by the influence of the factors studied, with the exception of vocabulary level and presence of an interceding inference. s2's responses were generally hypothetico-deductive and formal-operational, in contrast to s i ' s concrete syncretic reasoning. \ although both subjects' comprehension was affected by the level of vocabulary complexity, the subjects reacted differently to this factor. si resorted to a syncretic response, beforel attempting a strategy of overreliance on real world knowledge. s2 also ignored difficult lexical items but did not use primitive syncretic strategies. instead, he used a more appropriate strategy of imposing linguistic coherence by integrating what he had understood with his real world knowledge. with regard to the interceding inference factor, si made an incorrect interceding and final inference. s2's deduction of the interceding inference was influenced by residual preoperational difficulty with distinguishing real world from imaginary events (phillips, 1971). the relationship between imagination and inferential ability has not been addressed in the literature, but may be of interst for further investigation. conclusions the findings of this study suggested that the nature of learning-disability could be conceptualised in terms of a verbalcognitive interactional model. the study of inferential ability^permits generalizations about verbal and cognitive skills, since both verbal and cognitive knowledge are required to engage in inferential reasoning. cognition higher levels of logical reasoning i (i) u language higher level linguistic concepts verbal reasoning (ill u figure 3: interactional model of verbal-cognitive dynamics the main conclusion of this study was that the performance discrepancies documented, pointed to deficiencies in verbal-cognitive ability or 'verbal thought' (vygotsky, 1962) (see figure 3). the language-learning disabled child presented with:— — cognitive deficits that interfered with the growth of formal verbal reasoning and the acquisition of higher level linguistic concepts. this would be in agreement with bryen (1981) who asserted that "... this linguistic delay is not caused by inability to acquire certain linguistic symbols, but rather by reduction or delays in the acquisition of logical structures that determine their meaning". — linguistic deficits that possibly interfered with the development of higher levels of logical thinking and conceptual thought, as asserted by bruner (cited by lerner, 1976), and piaget (1967). however, the exact interactional dynamics between cognition and language in learning disabilities is still a topic of controversy as reflected in the following conclusion:— "the degree to which language delay contributes to or is a function of ... cognitive delay must still be determined" (gerber, 1981). however, it must be noted that a limitation of the study was the use of only 2 subjects which restricted a generalisability of the study. the study should be replicated on a larger sample in order to verify results. in addition, future research should address the issue of subjective evaluation by making use of more than one rater, and ensuring inter-rater reliability." this study highlighted the fact that comprehension is a multi-faceted process and that various aspects of this ability should be considered in therapy. the results of this study, when considered within the theoretical framework proposed earlier, suggest that therapeutic intervention could be systematically organized to focus on various components considered by the model of inferential ability described. this study outlined certain areas of particular importance for therapy with a patient with inferential deficits and strategic breakdown. therapy should consider such areas as schematic induction and accessing, a higher level linguistic deficits. therapy could focus on the comprehension of causal relations and other connective relationships. therapy should also stress comprehension of anaphoric devices and memory strategies (gerber, 1981; klein-konigsberg, 1984). therapy should emphasise lawful divergence and formulation of plausible hypotheses. finally, the therapist should stress the importance of the listener's perspective and encourage explicit reasoning and an analytical orientation. this study generated some implications for further research. developmental studies of inferential abilities in both normal and learning-disabled populations, are needed. other factors of the inferential comprehension model outlined earlier, may be investigated, such as the influence of level of abstraction, text organization or other categories of inference, on inferential strategies. the relationship between inferential ability and imagination has not been addressed in the literature, and constitutes an interesting area for investigation. the relationship between inferential comprehension and academic performance also needs to be considered in more depth, and the relationship between cognitive and linguistic deficits in learning disability warrants more attention. finally the construction of assessment tools for evaluating inferential comprehension skills, is required. the study of inferential ability holds interesting theoretical and practical implications for both speech therapists, educationalists and researchers. the continued investigation of this area is important, in view of its potential to clarify many controversial issues related to language pathology. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 references berry, μ. language disorders in children. appleton-century crofits, new york, 1969. bransford, j.d. and franks, j.j.: the abstraction of linguistic ideas. cognitive pshychology, 2, 3 3 1 3 5 0 , 1971. bryen, d.n. language and language problems. in: a. gerber and d.n. bryens (eds). language and learning disabilities. university park press, baltimore, 1981. carroll, d.w. psychology of language. brooks/cole publishing company, montery, california, 1986. crias, e.r. and chapman, r.s. story recall and inferencing skills in language learning-disabled and non-disabled children, journal of speech and hearing disorders, 52, 50—55, 1987. danner, f.w. and mathews, s.r. when do young children make inferences from pose? child development, 51, 906—908, 1980. dunn, l.m. peabody picture vocabulary test. american guidance service, minneapolis, 1965. ellis weismer, s. constructive comprehension abilities exhibited by language-disordered children. journal of speech and hearing research, 28, 1 7 5 1 8 4 , 1985. falstein, m. and miller, l.v. predicting outcomes: reading from comprehension. educational insights, compton, california, 1980. gerber, a. processing and use of language in education. in: a. gerber and d.n. bryen (eds). language and learning disabilities. university park press, baltimore, 1981 (a). gerber, a. remediation of langauge processing problems of the school-age child. in: a. gerber and d.n. bryen (eds). language and learning disabilities. univerity park press. baltimore, 1981(b). hammill, d.d., brown, v.l., larson, s.c. and wiederholt, j.l. test of adolescent language. pro-ed, austin, texas, 1980. johnson, m.k., bransford, j.d. and solomon, s.k. memory for tacit implications of sentences. journal of experimental psychology 98(1), 2 0 3 2 0 5 , 1973. kail, r.v., chi, m.t., ingram, a.l. and danner, f.w. constructive aspects of children's reading comprehension. child development, 48, 6 8 4 6 8 8 , 1977. klein-konigsberg, e. semantic integration and language learning disabilities: from research to assessment and intervention. in: g.p. wallach and k.g. butler (eds). language learning disabilities in school-age children. williams & wilkins, baltimore, 1984. lerner, j.w. children with learning disabilities: theories, diagnosis and teaching strategies. houghton mifflin company, boston, 1976. moates, d.r. and schumacher, g.m. an introduction to cognitive psychology, wadsworth publishing company, inc., belmont, california 1980. yael tombak and glenda shapiro nelson, n.w. beyond information processing: the language of teachers and textbooks. in: c.p. wallach & k.g. butler (eds). language learning disabilities in school-age children. williams and wilkins, baltimore, 1986. parades, k. drawing conclusions. frank schaffer publications, inc. california, 1980. paris, s.g. and carter, a.y. semantic and constructive aspects of sentence memory in children. developmental psychology, 9(1), 109-113, 1973. paris, s.g. and lindauer, b.k. the role of inference in children's comprehension and memory for sentences. cognitive psychology, 8, 2 1 7 2 2 7 , 1976. penn, c. a linguistic approach to the detection of minimal language dysfunction in aphasia. unpublished undergraduate research report, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, 1972. phillips, j.l. piaget's theory: a primer. w.h. freeman and company, san francisco, 1971. piaget, j. the language and thought of the child (1923). in: h.e. grohen and j.j. voneche (eds). the essential piaget. routledge & kegan paul, london, 1977. piaget, j. judgment and reasoning in the child (1927). in: h.e. groher and j.j. voneche (eds). the essential piaget. routledge & kegan paul, london, 1977. piaget, j. six psychological studies. university of london, 1967. reed, s.k. cognition: theory and applications. brooks/cole publishing company, montery, california, 1982. semel, e.m. and wiig, e.h. clinical evaluation of language functions. charles e. merrill publishing company, a. bell & howell company, columbia, ohio, 1980. thorndyke, p.w. the role of inferences in discourse comprehension. journal of verbal learning and verbal behaviour, 13, 4 3 7 5 0 6 , 1976. tombak, y. inferential comprehension strategies in a languagelearning disabled child. unpublished undergraduate research report, department of speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand, 1987. vygotsky, l.s. thought and language, 5 m.i.t. press, massachusetts, and john wiley & sons, inc., new york, 1962. westby, c.e. development of narrative language abilities. in c.p. wallach and k.g. butler (eds). language-learning disabilities in school-age children. williams & wilkins, baltimore, 1984. wiig, e.h. and secord, w. test of language competence. charles e. merrill publishing company, a. bell & howell company, columbus, ohio, 1985. wiig, e.h. and semel, e.m. language disabilities in children and adolescents. charles e. merrill publishing company, columbus, ohio, 1976. wiig, e.h. and semel, e.m. language assessment and intervention for the learning-disabled. charles e. merrill publishing company, a. bell & howell company, columbus, ohio, 1984. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparative study of inferential comprehension strategies appendix 1: test of language competence (wigg & secord, 1985) subtest 2: making inferences , > j5 to τ3 bo j3 qj t χ £ •c ™ ώα α α λ j3 g 3 qj l . a 3 £ > " ο oj c ! ι a a s 8 8 υ υ ο ϊ ΐ ι έ τ3 • > ο „ . ε β β i 8 a i § ο 53 -ό α. 1 3 « a •ΰ s-s g -w ο ο 5 =β » « s >, £ s ώ ? h d • cd ό j3 5 -) ιλ oj tfl u ί 3 ί 1/5 λ (λ re .b • • re •a " -π ο »η ϋ 60 α α 2 ^ c ο λ 0) w 3 .. c™ 3 e j3 j3 c qj λ 12 s qj 1) χ ε ε ο (λ s "s (λ qj χ § 2 ο 5 r ϋ a ω υ λ. έ ϊ ϋ to 0) > cao μο .3 •t? ™ 0j ο •π ξ π > sj5· t3 «>2 ™ -3 υ u υ c «» « u s-^o ά s ζ ο ί j3 •β •8 υ re 60 •ο τ3 τ3 {jj (η -c ε ο οο αϊ « αϊ .s .. c d ο λ 1 3 d λ co $ <0 s 5 ·* qj υ to 2 -ό to ra α ί u ϋ '13 mx ϋ 8·og α πί ε »j co χ i: dc e is <9 λ s (λ •fi ο i> s s " " s " κ ο iu .0 0 t: c ο ε ε i '5b 3 ο -c 0) oj 01 υ έ si t3 c (λ β ro oj "β > -r κ u β oj χ (λ aj ο 3 ο t , 3 a fr 3 β in « ω § j t § a l •a — £ s to " s s; | co ra 3 ο ϋ i ^ ^ oj g £ ^ 0) t3 0) "o "o ™ co co q ε •a τι ™ 5; έ j= 5 > >-. έ "3 >2 ξ jj m χ ι α .ο • < η . ·υ <υ ~ ε 2 ο ο iu 73 >η >-s « s >· c to . a "3. λ „ υ ο to j3 1 £ χ 3 ο ο w λ 2 -ο _ iu § | "ό 0) 0) <χ 0) ο •ϋ αϊ "2 — λ α; s αϊ χ η χ u υ τ3 • • 4) . •£ ό ^ .s 1 ρ 5 « qj ϊ ϋ 3 0j " co ν> λ 2 ε ο η ^ ο -ό co •π -ο to oj -ex ε η hq ε 2 qj oj a> j 5 ji • υ 'β, iu j3 1 ε — ° 2 ° o .. •ο 5p a s *j c « s i i l 3 '5 s> j3 co t3 i s s s s έ a s si ~ δ i s i ο t; , 2 .tl qj d ĵ h ch 3 c h o • μ § iu α 2 >< s § co w ε s h ό 3 si 0> ia λ χ bb £ •1 ε 1 1 1 . to q. ο jii ί3·ο g 1 • 1 $ o crt -ir crt o, " "d iu • λ i c 2 <υ α> s ε ε s t/ί qj c •s ω to xt . 2 μ i i s co v) 00 s.s c: 10 i s § c „ •α β =3 ? m 11 to 'θ ^ 53 co sd η 5 a > 0 ε « to „ 0) "73 •sag s 0 t3 l. —i qj t3 2 ο 1 1εξ§ j3 s o • die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 yael tombak and glenda shapiro appendix 2: test of inferential comprehension 1 a. inference of event 1. antecedent inference a. alan came home from school. he gave his mother the teacher's note. why had the teacher sent the note? b. paul went to the shops to buy yoghurt. he came home without the yoghurt. why did paul come home without the yoghurt? 2. interceding inference a. mrs ray received a dress by post. she had to take it back to the shop. why did mrs ray have to take the dress back? (adapted from paredes, k.: drawing conclusions, card 23(3). frank schaffer publications, inc., california, 1980). b. john had to pay a lot of money for the repairs. on his way home, he did not give the beggar any money. why didn't john give the beggar any money? 3. future inference a. mrs dean wanted a certain book about gardening. the bookshop did not have it. what do you think mrs dean will do? (adapted from paredes, k: drawing conclusions, card 27(2). frank schaffer publications, inc., california, 1980). b. greg pressed his face against the window. he wanted to see how the chefs made pizza. suddenly the window steamed up. what do you think greg will do? (adapted from paredes, k: drawing conclusions, card 27(2). frank schaffer publications, inc., california, 1980). b. inference of subjective state 1. reaction-based inference a. leah had a test. afterwards, she tore the test paper into little bits. how did leah feel about the test? b. dave teased eric. then, he pinched him. how did dave feel about eric? 2. stimulus-reaction based inference a. steve told his dad about the movie. his dad said he would go see it the next day. what did his dad think of steve's description of the movie? b. dad asked mat to bring his toolbox. "i left it over at larry's. he needed it." dad muttered something under his breath. how did dad feel about the toolbox being left at larry's house? (adapted from paredes, k.: drawing conclusions, card 27(2). frank schaffer publications, inc., california, 1980). 3. stimulus-based inference a. at last, merilyn had almost finished the painting. she had worked many months on it. all her friends praised her. how do you think marilyn will feel when she finishes the painting? (adapted from paredes, k.: drawing conclusions, card 15(2). frank schaffer publications, inc., california, 1980). b. shelly's aunt sent her a present. it contained an ugly pair of green socks. how do you think shelly will feel about the present? · appendix 3: test of inferential comprehension 2 a. inference of event 1. antecedent inference + vocabulary a. paul walked slowly down the hall, wondering what to do. as he stood before the door of the principal's office, he felt ashamed. he considered going home rather than facing miss conlin. she would be so disappointed in him. paul had always been an excellent student and he was never known to lie or cheat. it was only because his mother had been ill and he had to help take care of his brothers and sisters that he'd had to do it. paul took a deep breath and knocked lightly on the principal's door. miss colin called out "come in!" give some reasons why paul had been sent to the principal's office. (adapted from falstein & miller, 1980. predicting outcomes, cart 35: 'the best policy'.) b. as mr dean loped down the passage towards the manager's office, he felt that he was a despicable person. mr dean mounted the flight of stairs, considering all plausible alternatives as to his course of ensuing; action. he realised that the possibility of squirming jhis way out of this tricky situation was highly remote. the manager would be highly disappointed by his treacherous behaviour. mr dean had always been considered a jreputable employee and had never been known to engage in fraud, sabotage or forgery. it was only because of the dire financial straits of his family that he had felt compelled to participate. mr dean tapped briskly /on the manager's door, and he was told to enter. give some reasons why mr dean had been called to the manager's office. 2. interceding inference + schematic information a. granny jean was really proud of her grandchildren. she visited them almost everyday. she really enjoyed the visits to them. since the sun was shining, she decided to take a walk, rather than drive. spring was in the south african journal of communication disorders, vol.35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a comparative study of inferential comprehension strategies 15 the air, and the blossoms looked beautiful. by the time granny jean reached the house, the sky had clouded over. granny enjoyed the tea and thought the scones were particularly delicious. she soon felt very tired. granny realised that she would need to hurry since it was late and she still wanted to visit a friend of hers. she got up to leave. however, she realised that she would have to go back by taxi. give some reasons why she had to go back by taxi. b. uncle tim was the laziest man anyone had ever heard of. he was so lazy that he never walked anywhere. one day, uncle tim decided to visit a friend of his. as usual, he took his car. he always drove, since he was too lazy to walk. the drive was very enjoyable, and the gardens looked picturesque. uncle tim finally reached the house. he watched tv at his friend's house, and uncle tim thought that the western was particularly exciting. finally, uncle tim decided to leave, because he wanted to get to bed early. however, he soon realized that he would have to walk home. give some reasons why he had to walk home. 3. future inference ± reversible embedding a. marlene got a beautiful new watch for her birthday. she looked after it carefully and never let anyone touch it. lisa was her best friend. she loved beautiful things but no-one ever trusted her with anything. this was because lisa was a very careless girl. she didn't know how to look after things properly. although lisa begged marlene to lend her the watch, she always refused. one day, marlene and lisa went to the movies. marlene fell asleep. lisa decided to wear the beautiful watch for a few minutes even though it was to dark to see the time. : ι what do you think will happen next? the boy the girl knew, the boy had seen, had liked to collect stamps. the girl a stamp collection that grandfather had given to the family. the girl, the boy who liked stamps had tried |to persuade, never brought the stamps grandfather had given to school. one day, the friends convinced the girl the boy had seen, to bring the valuable stamp collection that grandfather had forbidden to take to school. the boy the girl knew, was very excited that they would see the stamps, that many collectors really wanted. the girl, the boy knew would bring the stamps, did not know how much the stamps were wanted by collectors. what do you think will happen next? b. inference in subjective state 1. reaction-based inference ± reversible passivization a. this is what michele was planning to do. as soon as shirley would look away, she would take her pencilbox. she would then keep her favourite ice cream rubber for her own collection. then she was planning to scatter all the other contents over the classroom. that would make shirley appear careless. then the teachers wouldn't believe her claim that someone may have stolen the beautiful rubber. in fact, they would all shout at shirley for being so careless and losing it. how did michele feel about shirley? why do you think so? b. a revenge had been planned by john. eric would be invited by john to a party. he would be given cold drinks by john. since the cold drinks would contain a sleeping powder, it would knock him out. eric would be carried by john to a friend's house. he would then be locked in a dark cupboard. eric would then be threatened by john. he would threaten to keep him there if he would not apologise. he would then be . made to promise never to tell anybody of what had happened. how did john feel about eric? why do you think so? 2. stimulus-reaction based inference ± microstructural aspect a. hannah henessee was a young woman whose husband was away in the army. she had to take care of the farm and the children. when hannah returned home one day, she discovered that the indians had kidnapped her children. they had carried the children away across the wide dangerous river. hannah swam across the deep water and walked into the indian camp. she demanded that the indians return her children. the indians were so surprised that they allowed her to take her children home. how did hannah feel about the indians? how did the children feel about hannah? (adapted from falstein & miller, 1980, predicting outcomes, card 37: ά revolutionary heroine'). i b. alex black was a young boy whose parents were away on holiday. relatives took care of the child in the meanwhile. everybody spoke of the highwaymen that had kidnapped many people. many caves in a nearby mountain provided a good place to keep hostages. a short trip by train led across desert towards the mountain. terrified faces met the entry of a young boy with his silver gun. soon, the group of hostages sped back to town by train. how did alex feel about the highwaymen? how did the hostages feel about alex? 3. stimulus-based inference ± intermediate inference a. when the don family visited the old ranch, roy was 7 the first'to explore the cellar. the most exciting thing he found that day was an old sheaf of papers containdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ing diary notes and maps. one of the maps was crumpled, faded and very interesting. this map contained directions for finding a treasure. it took him some time, but roy finally figured out the directions. he walked three steps to the left of the old farmer's barn, then he turned right and measured out three meters in that direction. finally he reached the spot from which he could see two apple trees. the directions said that if he would walk to the ditch behind the tree on the right and dig down two meters he would reach the treasure chest. roy had been digging for about ten minutes when his spade struck the lid of the chest. how will roy feel when he sees the contents of the chest? how will his family feel when they hear of the find? find? when the bascomb family moved into the old house, john was the first one to explore the attic. the most interesting thing he came upon that day was an old trunk filled with books and papers. one book's pages were cracked, yellow and particularly hard to read. this book contained a recipe for making gold. it took him a while, but john finally gathered all the ingredients. gold, after all, was something everyone could use more of. one day, while his family was out, john boiled twelve eggs. he then mixed them with manure from a white cat and added the juice of four grapefruits. he boiled that mixture for three hours. the recipe said that if any metal, such as tin or iron, was dipped into the mixture it would turn to gold. john had a tin can and three iron nails. how will john feel after he dips the metals in the mixture? how will his family feel when they hear what he had done? (adapted from falstein & miller, 1980, predicting outcomes, card 31: 'the alchemist'). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 33 communication abilities of non-standard language speaking children: a follow-up study erna alant, d. phil (pretoria) department of speech therapy and audiology, university of pretoria abstract there has recently been a growing awareness among speech and language pathologists about the problems of the non-standard language speaker when entering the school situation where standard language is predominantly used. this study deals with the preschool nonstandard language speaker and aims to investigate whether and to what extent the children's language and interaction patterns change after one year's exposure to a formal school situation. results indicate that although certain language skills do change, the functional interactions patterns of these children tends to remain the same. these findings are interpreted within a social context and implications for intervention discussed. opsomming daar is 'n groeiende bewuswording binne die geledere van die spraak en taalterapeute oor die probleme rakende die nie-standaardtaalspreker veral wanneer die nie-standaardtaalsprekende kind die skoolsituasie betree waar standaardtaal hoofsaakhk gebruik word. hierdie studie handel oor die voorskoolse nie-standaardtaalspreker en poog om na te vors of, en in welke mate die kmders se taal en interaksiepatroon verander na een jaar se blootstelling aan 'n formele skoolsituasie. resultate dui daarop dat hoewel sekere taalvaardighede verander het, die funksionele interaksie patroon van die kinders neig om meer konstant te bly. hierdie bevindings wordgemterpreteer binne 'n sosiale konteks en implikasies vir intervensie word bespreek. in the recent literature on language evaluation, much emphasis has been placed on the assessment of functional language, particularly in relation to non-standard language speakers. this pragmatic approach reflects an awareness of the difficulties involved iri comparing and describing communication abilities of children merely by looking at the formal structure of language (labov 1972, trudgill 1983, erickson and omark 1981). the inappropriateness of labelling the use of non-standarci language structures as 'pathological or deviant' due to inflexibility in the application of syntactic, phonological or other language rules, has contributed to an increasing consciousness of the equality of language variations as potential codes for the transmission of various kinds of messages (davis 1985, sturm 1984, edwards 1979, labov 1972). this acceptance of the equality of language variations (davis 1985), does not deny the existence of a standard language in society as represented by reading and writing. it acknowledges that societies need standard languages in order to function effectively, just as "schools have to accept and teach standard language if only because one' of the primary purposes of education is literacy" (davis 1985: 191). the consequence of schools aiming at familiarizing children with standard language as required for reading and writing, is the frequent existence of a 'mismatch' or 'discontinuity' between the language children use at home and at school (labov 1972, trudgill 1983, cox and jones 1983, adler 1979). die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 "... some children have little or no experience of the interactional demands of the school because of the type of conversational interaction which they have experienced at home, and so they are less able to cope with classroom talk" (mctear 1985:21). the nature of the discontinuity between home and school language is, however, not clear and could include a complex combination of social and linguistic aspects ranging from the use of different language structures to different communication styles (farran 1982). the question arises as to what extent children's language proficiency can change in coping with the more formal school situation after exposure to the educational system. it would be interesting to determine whether the language abilities and communication styles of children can be modified effectively in order to facilitate interaction at school after some experience with formal schooling. most of the longitudinal research on the language abilities of children exposed to schooling has been done by the american headstart programmes (moore 1979, kellaghan 1977), as well as the projects on the educational priority areas (epa) in britain (cox and jones 1983). these studies indicate that children who were part of these projects enjoyed only temporary advantages in adapting to the formal school situation and that they gradually drifted back towards the performance level of their companions who had no exposure to preschool education (moore 1979). explanations for these findings varied from ineffectively or poorly directed pro© sasha 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 erna alant grammes (kellaghan 1977), to the acceptance that language is part of a social structure and, therefore, that exposure to some hours of language or educational stimulation does not effectively change the interaction style of the individual (sturm 1984). the measurement of effective participation in a school context is, however, problematic in view of the difficulties involved in developing relevant communication parameters for indicating learning performance. school achievement gives some indication of children's ability to cope with scholastic demands, although only the results or outcome of learning behaviour is reflected. the process of learning itself is not considered in this kind of evaluation. in this respect it is pertinent to differentiate between "basic communication proficiency" which refers to interpersonal communicative skills and "cognitive academic language proficiency" which refers to the understanding and ability to manipulate meanings inherent in the language itself as proposed by cummins (1981) and discussed by skinner (1985). alhough these skills are interrelated, the nature of language proficiency required in different contexts varies in relation in the degree of context-dependency as well as cognitive demands (skinner 1985). different communication skills can therefore be identified in the formal learning situation, including interactive skills and the more cognitively orientated academic language skills. depending on the general approach to teaching, a more interactive (less authoritarian) or on the other hand, a representative interaction style (frequently associated with a more formal, authoritarian academic situation), could dominate. an interactive model of learning emphasizes the ability of the child to participate in the teaching situation by using a variety of language functions, i.e., to initiate, to respond, or to ask for clarification. interaction takes place primarily between pupil and teacher (coulthard 1977, britton 1973). formal instruction, however, also involves at least to some extent the ability to listen to relatively long and cognitively demanding verbal instructions or explanations in order to respond appropriately in the context, i.e., representational skills as defined by russel and russel (1979). different communication styles can therefore be identified in the teaching situation and should be noted when evaluating the communication abilities of children in the school situation (alant 1984). it is against this background that the present study sets out to investigate children's communication performance in two contexts: a more cognitively demanding representative context in which comprehension questions relating to a story are asked similar to the question-answer situation at school, and a less cognitively demanding conversational context where children and an adult simulate communication skills required in interaction with the teacher in the classroom. in view of this approach two questions demand to be posed, namely whether and to what extent the communication skills of children on a preschool level have changed after one year of exposure to the formal school situation and secondly, whether there is any significant association between overall school performance and communication ability as defined in the two contexts. methodology seventy-seven afrikaans-speaking children were tested on a preschool level. all of these were prospective schoolbeginners in the following year at a specific school in eersterust. the whole population of children that could be located was included in the study. twelve months later the same children, were retested at the end of their first school year. only forty-one of the original sample could be located at the school and were therefore included in the study. all the children tested were from the nantes area which is one of the poorer areas in eersterust, a community on the periphery of pretoria. it should be noted that, although close to the city, this township is relatively isolated from the larger afrikaans-speaking community in pretoria, and this has been contributed to the development of certain linguistic differences between the two afrikaans-speaking communities. these linguistic variations include differences in vocabulary, syntactic structures as well as pronunciation (claassen and van rensburg 1983). the average gross household income of people in eersterust was determined by a survey in 1979 (lotter, strijdom and schurink 1979) and reported to be less than r300 a month per sub-economical four-roomed house. as most of the housing in the nantes area is sub-economical, this could be taken as a reflection of the average income of this subdivision of the township. the average number of dependents per household was at least eleven (lotter et al. 1979:19). table 1 gives a brief description of the subjects used in the study. as the same children were tested on both occasions, only the relevant information will be given at the point of the second testing. table 1: description of subjects at the end of their first school year variable description number of children 41 male/female distribution 48,8% female; 51,2% male mean age 6,96 (sd = 0,53) scholastic achievement at the 6,96 (sd = 0,53) end of the first year 65% pass, 35% fail ι materials 1 ι the story: all children were exposed to a story and were required to answer questions about it. the same story and questions were used on both testing occasions. although the effect of exposure to the story during the first testing could have influenced performance on the task in the second testing, this influence was considered minimal. firstly, there was a twelve month break between the two testings, which generally is regarded as a sufficient period for retesting (dunn and dunn 1981). secondly, no answers were given to the children during the first testing situation, therefore no feedback was provided in terms of the correct answers. the story 'the fox and the crab' was'adapted from berry (1969) as modified by alant (1984) for use with five-and-ahalf year olds in a comprehension situation (see appendix 1). the story was lengthened for the purpose of testing comthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication abilities of non-standard language speaking children: a follow-up study 35 prehension by including more characters (factual material) while maintaining the basic story together with the inferences required to complete the ending. the story was told on video-tape (with facial clues and gestures only, in order to stimulate stories or narratives occurring in normal communication). a nursery school teacher as well as the teachers of the beginning classes at the school were asked to view the tape and to evaluate its appropriateness with regard to middle class biases in vocabulary, cultural prejudice, content, and age appropriateness. some minor changes were suggested after which the story was recorded for use in the pilot and main study. conversational themes: the conversational themes, were developed from the pilot study undertaken before the first testing. these topics centered around the children's interests and activities at school and at home, e.g., the family and favourite television programmes. procedure: each child was tested individually at school in a classroom specially equipped for the testing of children. during the first phase of the testing the children were required to observe the video-story (4 minutes) after which the comprehension questions were put to them. immediately after this interaction, the therapist having watched the video with the child, proceeded to initiate a conversation (12 minutes). the reason for this specific procedural sequence is that observation of the television story was considered to be less threatening to the children at the beginning of the interaction. the same therapist interacted with the children on both occasions in order to control for idiosyncratic communication differences. the complete contact with the child was recorded on video-tape for analysis at a later stage. however, the influence that the presence of the video equipment might have had on the performance of the children has to be considered. similarly the presence of a standard language speaker as a conversationalist could have had an inhibiting influence on the children. these two/factors were held constant during both testings in order to compare the children's behaviour in the same situation under similar circumstances one year later. j i verbal analysis: i , story situation: accuracy of answers: four questions were asked, based on the story (appendix 1). these questions were ranked from easy to difficult, that is, from questions demanding immediate reproduction of facts to questions relating to answers based on inferences drawn from the story. the grading of the questions was first tested by alant (1984) and proved to be satisfactory. the specific sequence of questions was deemed important as easy questions could be a motivating factor in the beginning of an interaction. probing: probing was used in order to prevent inhibited children from being discredited for lack of understanding. although the effectiveness of probing can be questioned, particularly with this sample of children (faegans and farran 1982, labov 1972), probes could contribute to an increase of verbal behaviour (stalnaker and creaghead 1982; warren, mcquarter and rogers-warren 1984). a probe was defined as a verbalization from the therapist followed by a pause (approximately one second) during which the child could be given an opportunity to respond. probing was used when children did not respond or when they responded with "don't know" or other short replies. non-directive probing was used at first, followed by more direct probes depending on the vagueness of the answers. functional analysis of conversation: the functional categories developed by dore (1977) were adapted for use in the analysis of conversation between the therapist (adult) and child (appendix 2). this analysis only describes the kinds of utterances used in the interaction (structural-functional) whereas the transactional aspects inherent in the semantic development of the conversation were not taken into account (mctear 1985). all the interviews were transcribed for use in the functional analysis. audi'o-recordings were used in order to enable the analyzers to do the functional analysis with the written as well as the audio-information. the inclusion of both transcript and audio-material in the analysis was necessary in order to facilitate reliable judgements of utterances, particularly in view of the fact that the function of an utterance cannot necessarily be deduced from the structure of the utterance (willes 1981). two groups of two analyzers each worked together so as to control their interpretations of the functional categories in order to increase reliability of the rating. an average of 96% agreement between raters was calculated for each interaction. scholastic achievement: an overall indicator of the children's academic achievement was obtained from the class teachers in the form of their final class mark. this mark constituted the average performance of the child on all the different levels, i.e., reading, writing, comprehension test, oral language and arithmetic. although it could be argued that scholastic achievement as defined above is too broad for comparison with the above testing procedure, language can be seen as an important, although not necessarily the only, factor influencing performance on all these levels. due to the complexity of factors that could influence school performance (e.g., visual problems) present findings should therefore only be interpreted tentatively. statistical analysis: descriptive measures such as means, standard deviations and standard error of the means were calculated to describe the performance of both groups on different variables. the means of specific variables were used so that certain features of the groups could be represented graphically. the die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 erna alant u-test of wilcoxon mann-whitney was used to test for differences in population means. results the results will be discussed in three broad categories: — the children's performance on the comprehension story; — the functional analysis of communication over the two testings; — the relationship between scholastic achievement and communication performance on the last testing (i.e., after the children had had eleven months' exposure to formal schooling). the children's performance on the comprehension story table 2 shows differences in the means of the children's performance on the different variables between the first (preschool) and second (school-going) testing. table 2: test for difference in means with respect to accuracy of response and probing needed to elicit responses variable t-value probability accuracy question 1 1,27 0,2098 question 2 4,74 0,0001»» question 3 2,87 0,0065»» question 4 2,88 0,0064»» total 4,91 0,0001»» probing question 1 0,64 0,5253 question 2 2,24 0,0305» question 3 -0,83 0,4119 question 4 1,18 0,2433 total 1,99 0,0537 ** = significant on 1% level, * = significant on 5% level. the above table indicates that there are significant differences between the means of most of the questions with respect to the accuracy of the responses, whereas there are relatively few significant differences on the amount of probing necessary to elicit responses from the children. functional analysis of conversation figures 1 and 2 graphically represent the mean number of utterances within each category for each testing. key: therapist x x x x x child: 3iack qo = open question qyln = yes/no question qs = specific question qc = question for clarification qh question repeated rvln = y :s/no response rf = factual response rc = clarifying response rr repeating response d description s 'statement aa acknowledgement, acceptance ap acknowledgement: positive an acknowledgement: negative oa organization device: attention op organization device: politeness oc organization device: contact ρ performatives int interruptions irr in elevant utterances qo qyln figure 1: functional analysis of conversations between therapist and child in 1984 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication abilities of non-standard language speaking children: a follow-up study x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x 15,48 x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x 8,05 x x x x x x x x x x 8,05 x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x 0,03 0,05 0,03 qo qyln qs key. therapist xxxxx child: black 24,05 x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x . x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x l 18 i x x x x x q» qyln qs qc qh ryln rl rc rr d s aa a p an oa op oc ρ inl irr open question yes/no question speeific question question for clarification question repeated yes/no response factual response clarifying response repeating response description statement acknowledgement: acceptance acknowledgement: positive acknowledgement: negative organization device: attention organization device: politeness organization device: contact performatives interruptions irrelevant utterances 1,25 2,05 2,5 x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x x 0,03 x x x x x 0,55 0,65 0,13 0,03 x x x x x 0,55 0,13 ap op figure 2: functional analysis of conversations between therapist and child in 1985 these figures indicate that there are no great differences between the therapist's and children's utterances for the two testing situations. table 3 reflects significant differences between the means on the different categories for the therapist's as well as the children's utterances. table 3: significant differences between means on categories used jby the therapist and child for the first and second testing table 4: test for the difference in means between the two groups (children that failed and passed) with respect to the story variables therapist child i probaprobacategory t-value bility category t-value bility qo 5,03 0,0001 φ φ qc -3,14 0,0037** d 2,37 0,0244 φ oa 3,58 0,0000 φ φ clarification of abbreviations in appendix ii . ** = significant on 1% level, * = significant on 5% level. correlation between overall scholastic achievement and communication parameters tables 4 and 5 represent the differences between the means of the story and conversational variables of two groups of children, group 1 representing the children who passed the first year of schooling (65% in total) and group 2 representing those who failed (35% in total). variable t-value probability accuracy question 1 (values were the same for accuracy one class level) question 2 -0,52 0,6065 question 3 -1,37 0,1778 question 4 -2,21 0,0336* total -2,20 0,0038* probing question 1 2,25 0,0326** question 2 1,27 0,2136 question 3 2,65 0,0120* question 4 1,27 0,2124 total 2,41 0,0210* significant on 1% level, * = significant on 5% level. from tables 4 and 5 it is clear that there are some differences between these two groups on the story variables, although fewer significant differences seem to be indicated between the first and second testing on the functional use of language. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 34, 1987 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 erna alant table 5: significant differences in means with respect to the functional categories used by the therapist and child therapist child category qr aa t-value 2,55 2,84 probability 0,02* 0,01** category ry t-value -1,94 probability 0,05* clarification of abbreviations in appendix ii ** = significant on 1% level, * = significant on 5% level. discussion of results children's performance on the comprehension story: accuracy of the responses: there is a highly significant difference in the children's accuracy of response between the first and second testing, reflecting definite growth in semantic understanding of the story during the twelve months. this improvement in the children's comprehension behaviour is reflected in questions two to four, with question one not showing any significant difference. this absence of a significant difference on question one can be expected in view of the inherent nature of the question as it simply demands the name of the animal which is the main character of the story. the highly significant difference between the means on question two is, however, interesting considering the demands made on memory and recall of the children as they were required to name all the animals that the fox met. this finding is in agreement with developmental research reporting increased ability to remember factual detail as age increases (bloom and lahey 1978). question three indicates the ability to analyse the purpose of the actions in the story by describing the plan that the fox had. the significant improvement in answers on this question is in congruence with the research done by peterson and mccabe (1983) where they emphasize the older child's tendency to move away from describing action sequences to expressing relationships (cause — effect or intentionality) between actions. they explain this phenomenon in terms of the children's increase in control over their environment in that they become better able to participate in events whereas the purpose of these events might not have been evident to them before. question four emphasizes the ability to constructively process information by demanding that the children make inferences from the story. the present data confirm normal developmental research reporting an age effect in the constructive processing of information (small and butterworth 1981, paris, lindauer and cox 1977). weissmer (1983) also reported fewer inferences from children aged 5,5 — 6,7 compared to those aged 7,7 — 9,2. in general the language performance of these children indicates an increased ability to cope with the two main language proficiency vectors as formulated by cummins (1981), i.e., the ability to determine and communicate meaning in the absence of contextual clues and the ability to think in more 'cognitively demanding' situations. although this improved performance could also be interpreted as the result of the children's familiarity with the testing situation it is doubtful whether this is the case particularly in view of the twelve month gap between testings. probing necessary to elicit responses: only question two indicates a significant difference (at the 5% level) between the means of the two testings, implying that more probing was done on the second than on the first testing. this finding is contrary to what is expected as it implies that the children needed more encouragement to answer the questions on the second testing. this finding raises questions as to the influence of probing on the accuracy scores of the children. the association between probing and accuracy for this particular procedure has, however, been computed, and indicates a highly significant neg,.:t die eerste twee tendense kan aanduidend wees van adaptasie met herhaalde produksie wat 'n kenmerkende simptoom van apraksie is (darley, aronson en brown 1975). die ander tendense is slegs 'n weerspie'eling van die onkonstante produksie van die apraktiese spreker. 43 figuur 1: grafiese voorstelling van die zwaarde by die apraktiese spreker. 10 r 5 -· 4,14 2,60 , 0 , 3 7 / s a t / figuur 1.1 1 0 7,62 -.4,72 2,67 / s a n / figuur 1.2 10 -2,52 . . 1,25 /sfcnt/ figuur 1.3 10 5 -• 10,98 . . 2,74 1,52 / s a n t / figuur 1.4 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 1 , 9 0 /kat/ figuur 1.5 / t a n / figuur 1.6 a a n t / figuur 1.7 matilda de j a g e r 10 -0 , 9 6 1 0 . . 5 -• 2,12 1 , 5 5 1 , 8 4 / l e n t / figuur 1.8 / f a l k / figuur 1.11 1 0 4 , 8 9 / f a k / figuur 1.9 2 5 j2 0 1 5 . . . 2 4 , 7 4 1 0 -/ f a l / figuur 1.10 10 5 . 4 , 5 0 / f a l k / figuur 1.12 1 0 5 -8 , 4 5 4 , 2 9 2 , 0 7 / p a k / figuur 1.13 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a s p e k t e van tydreeling in normale en verbaal-apraktiese spraak 1 0 5 . . 2 , 2 4 1 , 5 4 1 , 4 3 /pal/ figuur 1.14 1 2 , 8 7 2 , 4 0 uit bogenoemde is dit dus duidelik dat daar onkonstantheid bestaan ten opsigte van die segmentele tydsduur in die herhaalde produksie van 'n woord deur die proefpersoon met verbale apraksie. onkonstantheid en variasie van die foutpatroon oor herhalings van dieselfde woord is een van die klinies mees waarneembare en kenmerkende eienskappe van apraktiese spraak (kent & rosenbek, 1983). in teenstelling daarmee produseer die normale spreker elke herhaling met bykans dieselfde duur vir elke segment. 'n verdere verskynsel wat waargeneem word, is dat die z-waarde van die verskillende woorde soos geproduseer deur die apraktiese spreker, verskil. die z-waarde is aanduidend van die mate waarin 'n produksie van die normale afwyk. volgordelike rangskikking van die z-waardes is moontlik en word voorgestel in tabel 4. die afleiding wat moontlik hieruit gemaak kan word, is dat hoe hoer die z-waarde is, hoe meer afwykend is die produksie en dat die moeilikheidsgraad van die produksie van die verskillende woorde varieer by die apraktiese spreker. deur die afleiding te maak dat 'n die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 45 moeilike produksie meer afwykend sal wees as 'n makliker produksie, kan daar op grond van die z-waardes bepaal word watter spesifieke produksies vir die spesifieke spreker makliker of moeiliker is, byvoorbeeld vir die w o o r d e / ' / f ^ / en /falk/ is die gemiddelde z-waardes onderskeidelik 7,31 en 1,83. daar kan nie regstreeks gese word dat dit vir die apraktiese spreker makliker is om 'n / a / as//o/ te produseer nie want z-waarde vir /fal/ is 13,25 wat dus weer hoer is as die z-waarde vir / f o l k / . tabel 4: volgordelike rangskikking van die mate waarin geproduseerde woorde van die apraktiese spreker afwyk van die normale soos aangedui deur die z-waarde van elke produksie. daar kan wel afgelei word dat dit vir hierdie spreker makliker is om 'n / a / te produseer in kombinasie met 'n inisi'ele /{/ en finale / l k / as wat dit vir hom is om 'η /=/ te produseer in dieselfde klankomgewing. hierdie feit ondersteun die stelling dat persone met verbale apraksie dikwels probleme ondervind met 'n spesifieke klank wanneer dit in 'n spesifieke klankomgewing voorkom (van der merwe, 1985). deur gebruik te maak van hierdie metode van ontleding waarin die z-waarde vir elke produksie addisioneel tot ouditiewe analise van simptome bepaal word, kan daar dus met groter akkuraatheid vasgestel word presies waar die spesifieke pasient se probleem le. veral in 'n gevorderde stadium van terapie kan so 'n analise waardevolle terapieleidrade verskaf. die onkonstantheid ten opsigte van kv as persentasie van totale duur by herhaling van dieselfde woord deur die apraktiese spreker word verder gei'llustreer deur tabel 5. hierin word aangetoon dat die gemiddelde konstantheidsvariasie vir proefpersone 1 tot 4 onderskeidelik 3,43%; 3,97%; 4,29% en 4,13% is. die normale omvang is dus 3,43% tot 4,29% en die gemiddelde konstantheidsvariasie-persentasie vir die normale proefpersone is 3,95%. by die apraktiese spreker is die gemiddelde konstantheidsvariasiepersentasie 10,97% wat dus ver buite die normale omvang en baie hoer as die gemiddeld van die normale is. dit dui dus daarop dat daar groter variasie is ten opsigte van die kv-persentasie van totale duur by herhaling van dieselfde woord deur die apraktiese spreker as by die normale spreker. hierdie variasie ten opsigte van kv as persentasie van totale duur dui weer eens op die onkonstantheid en variasie in die produksies van die apraktiese spreker. onkonstantheid en variasie is egter ook kenmerkend van kinderspraak. sharkey en folkins (1985) haal disimoni (1974b) en tingley en allen (1975) se studies aan waarin gevind is dat die varieerbaarheid van baie van die parameters van spraak afneem met ouderdom tot by puberteit. nadat 'n kind geleer het om 'n taak suksesvol uit te voer, word die onderliggende motorprosesse verfyn deur verhoogde antisiperende komponente van die r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 matilda de j a g e r tabel 5· vergelyking tussen die normale sprekers en die apraktiese ' spreker ten opsigte van kv%-konstantheid oor 3 herhalings van dieselfde woord. voorstelling deur middel van konstantheidsvariasie-waardes. 3 g •c u i woorde sat san sant sent kat kan kant k nt fak fal falk folk pak pal palk gemiddelde kon· stall theidsvariasie vir elke proefpersoon proefpersoon 1 1,68 4,95 4,60 0,98 3,10 1,82 0,78 4,82 4,29 5,92 6,28 3,02 3,08 3,22 2,91 3,43% proefpersoon 2 0,87 1,93 2,91 4,02 6,77 3,37 2,71 4,68 2,53 6,14 6.73 3.74 10,61 0,81 1,76 3,97% proefpersoon 3 7,86 0,96 1,05 0,25 1,66 1,40 2,27 2,68 23,02 0,71 5,17 3,76 1,70 3,38 8,55 4,29% proefpersoon 4 0,76 3,11 8,95 2,15 3,69 4,92 2,69 9,63 0,56 4,74 3,76 4,65 5,42 3,88 3,11 4,13% ap 6,16 11,66 12,64 13,64 3,87 14,35 19,28 9.98 12,86 5,20 7.99 23,08 4,20 11,35 8,32 10,97% omvang van normale konstantheidsvariasie 3,43%-4,29% gemiddelde konstandheidsvariasie-persentasie vir normale proefpersone 3,95% tabel 6 toon verder aan dat die kv-persentasie van dieselfde woord baie groter is by die apraktiese spreker as by die normale sprekers. byvoorbeeld vir die woord / k a t / is die kv-persentasie vir die apraktiese spreker gemiddeld 65% en vir die normale sprekers slegs 44,30%. uit die roudata was dit duidelik dat segmentele tydsduur oor die algemeen baie langer is by die apraktiese spreker as by die normale sprekers. hierdie verlengde segmentele tydsduur resulteer dus in stadiger produksie. dit is egter ook opvallend dat die vokaal in elke woord heelwat meer verleng word as die konsonante in dieselfde woord. dit bevestig kent en rosenbek (1983) se bevindings dat vokaalverlenging en stadige produksie kenmerkend is van apraktiese spraak. die hoe kv-persentasie wat voorkom by die apraktiese spreker is dus die gevolg van die oormatige verlenging van die vokaal. die persentasie wat die kk uitmaak van die totale duur van die woord is dus nou korter by die apraktiese spreker as by die normale sprekers. dit moet egter nie gei'nterpreteer word as dat die apraktiese spreker die kk vinniger produseer nie. die duur in millisekondes van die kk is steeds langer as die normale duur. die persentasie wat dit van die totale woordduur uitmaak, is egter kleiner omdat die kv-gedeelte, as gevolg van oormatige vokaalverlengings, so 'n groot persentasie van die totale woordduur beslaan. tabel 6: vergelyking tussen normale sprekers en 'n apraktiese spreker ten opsigte van die kv-persentasie ap = apraktiese proefpersoon. woorde omvang van kvpersentasie by normale sprekers gemiddelde kvpersentasie vir die normale sprekers kv-persentasie by die apraktiese spreker / s a t / 63,75 68,74 67,13 72,59 / s a n / 61,21 68,40 64,80 78,92 / s a n t / 5 1 , 2 5 6 1 , 6 3 55,81 69,31 / s t i l t / 48,56 64,00 56,33 65,77 / k a t / 41,90 45,90 44,30 65,00 / k a n / 46,86 52,62 49,56 69,80 / k a n t / 34,41 39,18 37,49 53,44 a s n t / 27,35 38,81 31,71 42,03 / f a k / 54,56 61,30 57,79 67,33 / f a l / 62,06 63,63 62,78 81,33 / f a l k / 43,63 57,45 53,54 65,36 / f o l k / 43,62 55,05 50,52 61,53 / p a k / 39,81 4 9 , 5 1 45,55 60,39 / p a l / 28,12 57,01 48,36 71,14 / p a l k / 31,00 37,89 35,46 51,32 bewegingskema, deur meer ekonomiese gebruik van energie en deur minder variasie by die herhaling van 'n taak (sharkey & folkins, 1985). di simoni (1974b) het byvoorbeeld gevind dat kinders eers na 9 jaar die temporale reel van temporale kompensasie aanleer, met ander woorde, verfyning geskied ook op die gebied van tydreeling. dit wil dus voorkom asof hierdie beheer weer verlore gaan by die apraktiese spreker en resulteer in onkonstantheid in die toepassing van tydreeling. dit is dus moontlik om die afleiding te maak dat tydreelingvariasie wat in apraktiese spraak voorkom, ooreenstemming kan toon met die tydreelingvariasie in die spraakproduksie van kinders. waar die kind nog nie volkome beheer oor tydreeling ontwikkel het nie, het die apraktiese spreker moontlik weer die beheer verloor. kent en forner (1980) stel ook voor dat groter variasie in kinderspraak verband kan hou met die feit dat hulle spraak stadiger is as die van volwasse spraak en dat stadige spraak predisponerend is tot verhoogde tydreelingsfoute. stadige produksie is ook kenmerkend van apraktiese spraak (kent & rosenbek, 1983) en kan dus saam met foutiewe programmering die oorsaak wees van die variasie in die toepassing van tydreeling by die apraktiese spreker. in die produksie van die aprakties spreker word dieselfde tendens as by die normale spreker waargeneem, naamlik dat 'n kontinuant plus 'n vokaal in kombinasie met / n t / 'n groter kvpersentasie het as 'n ploffer plus 'n vokaal in kombinasie met / n t / (sien tabel 3). daar kan dus afgelei word dat hierdie temporale reel by hierdie spesifieke apraktiese spreker behoue gebly het. i i alhoewel die produksie van die apraktiese spreker uitermate verleng word relatief tot normale produksie en dus dui op afwy kende programmering van segmentele tydsduur, demonstreer verskeie studies dat duurverhoudings wel behoue bly by hierdie sprekers. collins, rosenbek en wertz (1983) bestudeer die verhouding tussen vokaalen woordduur by normale en apraktiese sprekers in langerwordende woorde. die resultate toon dat die vokaal korter word in verhouding met die duur van die basiswoord namate die woord langer word by die normale en by die apraktiese sprekers. collins et al. (1983) haal ook vir freeman, sands en leavitt aan wat demonstreer dat die engelse fonologiese reel, naamlik dat vokale langer is voor 'n stemhebbende as voor 'n stemlose konsonant, ook toegepas word deur apraktiese sprekers. uit bogenoemde kan daar dus afgelei word dat sekere aspekte van temporale programmering, byvoorbeeld segmentele tydsduur, the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a s p e k t e van tydreeling in normale en verbaal-apraktiese spraak afwykend is by die apraktiese spreker terwyl ander, byvoorbeeld duurverhoudinge, intakt is. gevolgtrekkings met betrekking tot die normale sprekers daar bestaan 'n neiging tot 'n konstante kv-persentasie oor drie herhalings van dieselfde woord by 'n individu. hierdie konstantheid is egter nie absoluut nie omdat daar motoriesekwivalente bewegingspatrone in spraak bestaan. hieruit kan ook afgelei word dat temporale kompensasie wel by afrikaanssprekende proefpersone voorkom en bydra tot die neiging van 'n konstante kv-persentasie oor herhalings van dieselfde woord. daar bestaan nie 'n neiging tot 'n konstante kv-persentasie in kvkk-eenhede byvoorbeeld / s a t / teenoor / s a n t / nie. die temporale reel in engels, naamlik dat die duur van die kv in 'n kvken kvkk-eenheid ongeveer dieselfde bly in verhouding tot die totale duur (walsh 1984), kom dus nie by die afrikaanssprekende proefpersone voor nie. konsonantkombinasieverkorting kom dus in 'n mindere mate in afrikaans voor as byvoorbeeld in engels en dus nie met die doel om vasgestelde duurverhoudinge te behou nie. -verskillende vokale het 'n geringe invloed op die kvpersentasie van die totale duur van kvkk-eenhede met dieselfde inisiele konsonant en eindkonsonantkombinaise. die kv-persentasie word dus bepaal deur die klankomgewing waarin dit voorkom. uit die toetsmateriaal kan afgelei word dat daar 'n temporale reel in afrikaans bestaan, naamlik dat 'n kontinuant plus 'n vokaal 'n groter persentasie van die woord uitmaak as 'n plotter plus 'n vokaal wanneer dit geproduseer word voor die konsonantkombinasie / n t / . met betrekking tot die apraktiese spreker deur ontleding van 'n apraktiese spreker se spraak in terme van segmentele tydsduur en die bepaling van 'n z-waarde vir elke produksie, kan daar in 'n groot mate bepaal word met watter klanke in watter spesifieke klankomgewing die spreker die meeste probleme het. deur hierdie metode van analise 'addisioneel tot ouditiewe aiialise van apraktiese simptome toe te pas, kan waardevolle terapieleidrade veral in 'n gevorderde stadium van behandeling ^erkry word. daar bestaan onkonstantheid en variasie van die foutpatroon ten opsigte van segmentele tydsduur in die herhaalde produksie van 'n woord by die apraktiese proefpersoon. i daar bestaan onkonstantheid van die kv-persentasie met die herhaling van dieselfde woord. daar is dus groter variasie ten opsigte van die kv-persentasie by herhaling van dieselfde woord deur die apraktiese spreker as by die normale spreker. variasie van tydre'eling in die produksie van die apraktiese spreker kan moontlik verband hou met die tydreelingsvariasie in kinderspraak. ι die temporale reel wat by normale sprekers voorkom, naamlik dat 'n kontinuant plus 'n vokaal in kombinasie met / n t / 'n groter kv-persentasie het as 'n ploffer plus 'n vokaal in kombinasie met / n t / , het behoue gebly by hierdie apraktiese spreker. die kv-persentasie by die apraktiese spreker is deurgaans groter as vir die normale sprekers en dit bevestig kent en 47 rosenbek (1983) se bevindinge, naamlik dat vokaalverlenging en stadige produksie kenmerkend is van apraktiese spraak. aangesien hierdie slegs 'n eerste poging was om die programmering en toepassing van tydreeling in afrikaans te bestudeer, bestaan daar nog 'n groot behoefte aan verdere kennis en navorsing op hierdie gebied. bedankings die skryfster wil graag mev. a. van der merwe, departement spraakheelkunde en oudiologie, universiteit van pretoria, bedank vir haar leiding met die uitvoer van die studie waarop hierdie artikel gegrond is. verwysings bell-berti, f. en harris, k.s. a temporal model of speech production. phonetica, 38, 9-20, 1981. collins, m.; rosenbek, j.c. en wertz, r.t. spectrographic analysis of vowel and word duration in apraxia of speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 26, 224-230, 1983. cooper, m.h. en allen, g.d. timing control accuracy in pormal -speakers and stutterers. journal of speech and hearing research, 20, 55-71, 1977. darley, f.l.; aronson, a.e. & brown, j.r. motor speech disorders. w.b. saunders company, philadelphia, 1975. disimoni, f.g. influence of consonant environment on duration of vowels in the speech of three, six, and nine-year old children. j. acoust. soc am, 55, 362-363, 1974a. disimoni, f.g. some preliminary observations on temporal compensation in the speech of children. j. acoust. soc. am, 56, 697-699, 1974b. disimoni, f.g. evidence for a theory of speech production based on observations of the speech of children. j. acoust. soc. am, 56, 1919-1921, 1974c. disimoni, f.g. en darley, f.l. effect on phoneme duration control of three utterance length conditions in an apraxic patient. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 42, 257-264, 1977. kent, r.d. en forner, l.l. speech segment durations in sentence recitations by children and adults. journal of phonetics, 8,157168, 1980. kent, r.d. en rosenbek, j.c. prosodic disturbance and neurologic lesion. brain and language, 15, 159-192, 1982. kent, r.d. en rosenbek, j.c. acoustic patters of apraxia of speech. journal of speech and hearing research, 26, 231-249, 1983. lofqvist, a. en yoshioka, h. interarticular programming in obstruent production. phonetica, 21-34, 1981. sharkey, s.g. en folkins, j.w. variability of lip and jaw movements in children and adults: implication for the development of speech motor control. journal of speech and hearing research, 8-15, 1985. tingley, b.m. en allen, g.d. development of speech control in children. child development, 46, 186-194, 1975. van der merwe, a. terapieprogram vir verbale ontwikkelingsapraksie met toepassingsmoontlikhede vir ander spraakafwykings, pretoria: universiteit van pretoria, 1985. walsch, t, modelling temporal relations within english syllables. journal of phonetics, 12, 19-35, 1984. walsch, t. en parker, f. vowel length and "voicing" in a following consonant. journal of phonetics, 9, 305-308, 1981. walsch, t. en parker, f. consonant cluster abbreviation: an abstract analysis. journal of phonetics, 10, 423-437, 1982. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) adm\nced for tomorrow. the μ α κ ό m a 4 1 progressive. the latest technology* built to meet tomorrow^ needs head-on. that's the maico ma41. it's a portable audiometer with reliable circuitry and features that you require in an office audiometer. like speech and narrow band masking, talk forward and talk back, and the new b71 bone vibrator. t h e ma41 is sophisticated enough for all applications, yet it's remarkably easy to use. so you can concentrate on your patient. n o t the machine. t h e maico ma41. a top-of-the-line portable that's advanced for t o m o r r o w . . . but available today. from maico ^ r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) partners in basic education.html partners in basic education the publication of this special edition of the south african journal of communication disorders, ‘speech-language therapists and audiologists as partners in basic education’, contributes to our transformation. it is part of a series of other activities under the auspices of the south african speech-language-hearing association, aiming to reposition our professions so they can contribute effectively to development in south africa. at the 2010 saslha conference, ‘new directions’, there were several inputs highlighting the national crisis in basic education and the potential role the professions could play in improving education outcomes. the conference led to formation of the education task team, who considered a series of activities to advance our future practice. it was recognised that interventions in basic education must be relevant to the south african context and that new and different knowledge was needed. as a first step it was imperative to document the current issues and available knowledge – hence the idea for this dedicated special edition of sajcd. it is envisaged that research in the domain of basic education will continue to grow as we shape our practices in a rapidly changing social milieu. we are also aware that south africa is part of a global community that must participate in the international dialogue on practice. we have therefore invited professor barbara ehren, an expert in school-based interventions, to provide a response to the lead article which contextualises the argument for renewed roles for speech-language therapists and audiologists in basic education in south africa. we recognise that we are at the beginning of a seemingly daunting task, but believe that an accumulation of relevant knowledge as well as strategic actions on our part will result in positive contributions to nation-building. harsha kathard editor: special edition sajcd anatomical and spectrographic analysis of the voice in disease: a report of five cases w.a. k e r r , m.r.c.s., d.l.o., department of otorhinolaryngology, johannesburg hospital. (head: d.r. haynes) and l.w. l a n h a m , ph.d. head, department of phonetics & general linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg summary five cases are presented. one is a case of ventricular p h o n a t i o n of iatrogenic origin and the remaining four had undergone l a r y n g e c t o m y for carcinoma of the larynx. points of interest are discussed, particularly the constant ventricular fold p h o n a t i o n of the first case and t h e clear harmonic structure present in the voice of o n e of the l a r y n g e c t o m y cases w h o has b o t h esophageal speech and pharyngeal p h o n a t i o n . opsomming v y f gevalle w o r d voorgele. die een is 'n geval van ventrikulere fonasie van iatrogeniese oorsprong, terwyl die o o r b l y w e n d e vier gevalle laringektomie ondergaan het vir karsinoma van die larinks. a s p e k t e van belang w o r d bespreek, veral die t e e n w o o r d i g h e i d van harmoniese struktuur in die stem van die eerste geval m e t ventrikulere fonasie, a s o o k in die stem van een van die laringektomie-gevalle wat b e i d e esofageale en faringeale fonasie gebruik. the authors do know that much has been written on this subject and that a comprehensive reference to the many authors would be impossible in this article. but they have recorded some publications in the list of references and acknowledge the work of such authorities as: negus, c. and c.l. jackson, kallen, bateman, kirchner, arnold, huizinga, van den berg, moore, norris, conley, tapia, tato, moolenaar bijl and the bell telephone company. this paper is comprised of two sections first (part i), the discussion of the spectrographic evidence, second (part ii), the anatomical analysis and case histories. editor's note: we gratefully acknowledge a grant-in-aid for the publication of this article from the national cancer association of south africa. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 w.a. kerr and l.w. lanhamlll-λλ1 j journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) anatomical and spectrograph analysis of the voice in disease 83 it is hoped that the association of the disciplines of acoustic phonetics and otorhinolaryngology, as well as the method of presentation, may be of interest in south africa; and, if so, this report will be a fitting tribute to professor p. de v. pienaar who has worked for many years in this country in speech therapy on the foundations of phonetics. part i the physical analysis of the different "voices". "voice" refers to the manner in which the upper vocal tract is made to function as a resonating system bringing out the distinctive qualities of vowels and vowel-like speech sounds (i.e. oral and nasal resonants such as 1 and r, m and n), whose qualities depend on the resonances of the vocal tract. in this section of the paper the acoustic properties of the different voices are analysed in an attempt to correlate this analysis with states of the esophageal sphincter, the pharynx and the organs of normal speech. the acoustic properties are extracted by means of a spectrograph^ analysis. the measurement of pharyngeal pressure is resorted to in one case in order to confirm pharyngeal constriction. in physical properties each of the voices of the five cases are different in their own way and are examined in the order of, first, the case of ventricular phonation followed by the four cases of esophageal speech including case d, an interesting case who has two distinct "voices". the aim of this analysis is to identify features which may contribute to a classificatory framework of voice without vocal folds, and to an understanding of some of the compensatory mechanisms involved in producing intelligible speech. spectrographic anal ysis of speech sounds the kay sonagraph model 6061-b was used to provide spectrograms showing the acoustic properties of the speech sounds made by our five cases. the spectrograms show frequency on the vertical axis from 0-8 khz., amplitude in varying degrees of darkness in lines and smudges made by the stylus of the sonagraph, and frequency/amplitude changes in time on the horizontal axis (12,33 cm = 1 sec.). the sonagraph provides fine-grained analysis on the vertical axis in a narrow-band display in which the instrument registers conflated intensity in bandwidths of 45 cps; variations in frequency over very brief intervals are therefore registered separately. the wide-band display conflates intensity over bandwidths of 300 cps. wide-band analysis provides fine-grained analysis in the time dimension. the analysis of variations in time in narrowband spectrograms is coarse, as are variations in frequency in broad-band displays. in the analysis of speech sounds which depend on the glottal note, a narrowband spectrogram highlights harmonic structure. the spectrogram marked normal ii shows a harmonic structure with a fundamental of approximately 120 cps in the region; of 19 to 20 on the horizontal cm scale. the resonances of the supra-glottal vocal tract amplify, selectively, harmonics in the glottalnote and these amplified harmonics are seen to be darker and thicker at a/b tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 84 w.a. kerr and l.w. lanham whisper i "(wide) i i 12 13 i "» , i (1 3! her, (high pitch) 5 , , a , , , , 0 i 11 ι \ ζ ι i 3 ι 11 . 1 : h her (1ow pi tch) whisper ii (narrow) 11 12 13 4 i s i ii i / « a i 3 i 1 ο i 1 1 > i 2 ' 1 3 i 1 => ι ! ̂ ί 1 s. i 1 7 i 1g 1 i ο 1 journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) anatomical and spectrograph analysis of the voice in disease 85 and c in the vertical scale. resonance bars appear much more clearly at equivalent points on the wide-band normal i spectrogram. wide-band spectrograms highlight resonance properties on which the distinctive quality of vowels and resonant consonants depend and such resonance properties are the products of the cavities of the supra-glottal vocal tract. wide-band displays obliterate harmonic structure. resonance bars are normally termed "formants" and the formant structure for the vowel [3:] of ken emerges as f1 = 550, f2 = 1340, f3 = 2250 (approximate centre frequencies of lowest three formants). fine-structure analysis revealing the pulses of the glottal note shows in wide-band spectrograms in the thin parallel vertical lines rising vertically through most of the visible frequency scale where vowels are articulated. each of the highly regular (in time and amplitude) pressure pulses from the vibrating vocal folds is shown by a vertical line which also thickens and darkens as it enters the bandwidth of a formant. glottal pulses up to 500 per second are discernible in wide-band spectrograms. aperiodic noise, at whatever point in the vocal tract it is created, shows as striations in wide-band displays: i.e. as irregular vertical lines of varying lengths as seen at normal i 10. here the consonant [s] is seen as randomly distributed energy concentrated mainly above 3.5 khz which, in time, has a relatively gradual onset and continues for approximately 16 csecs. noise in the form of a burst, i.e. instantaneous onset and rapid decay is seen at case ai 7 corresponding to the release of the [t] of to. aperiodic high-intensity noise is also clearly shown by whisper i where random, irregular frequency/ amplitude components are seen to be amplified by resonances; compare the ill-defined, but discernible, formant 2 of [3:] in her in whisper i and [3:] of ken in normal i. an aperiodic noise component in narrow-band spectrograms appears as a horizontally elongated smudge rather than a thin vertical line. the representation of noise in the two different spectrographs samplings are clearly shown by whisper i (wide band) and whisper ii (narrow band). the means of identifying areas on the spectrograms presented here is in terms of coordinates on the horizontal and vertical scales. in illustration, the square box in the centre of spectrogram whisper i is 7 to 8 g to j. the title over each spectrogram should be interpreted thus: case dv esophageal (wide) is a wide-band spectrogram identified by the number v and representing the voice of case d who has esophageal voice. note that v and vi are wide and narrow-band spectrograms of the same utterance. the means of interpreting the pharyngeal pressure graphs are discussed below. voice types in this study the five cases range, in their speech, from high to relatively low levels of intelligibility (with some fluctuation in individual cases) and the correlation between these differences with the different means of exciting the resonators is attempted below. resonators function in response to an input in which a driving force is involved. in the human voice the driving force is always an air flow which passes a point of constriction and the resonators of the upper vocal tract can be excited in three different ways. the air flow may be interrupted at the point of tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse verenigingvir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 86 w.a. ker and l.w. lanham case all ventricular (narrow) _ ρ parents to see doctor kerr journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) anatomical and spectrograph analysis of the voice in disease 87 constriction by rapidly alternating closed and open phases. in normal voice lung air is interrupted in highly regular and rhythmical vibratory cycles by the true vocal folds which are drawn medially and, in varying degrees, laterally, into a state of vibration. these vibrations are a consequence of the bernoulli effect in which an accelerating air flow through a narrowing channel sucks the elastic edges of the constrictor together. closure brings a change in pressure at the point of constriction and is followed by rapid opening. the vibrations of normal phonation set up pressure pulses which are highly regular in time and amplitude and only vibrators having the properties of the vocal folds, violin strings, etc. can produce a stream of pressure pulses of this kind. these pulses create a periodic sound wave with an inherent harmonic structure, i.e. the energy in the vibrations is largely concentrated at points in the frequency scale which are multiples of the fundamental (the lowest frequency component). very little turbulence occurs in this type of interrupted air flow even as the closed phase of the vibratory cyclc is approached and there is, therefore, little concomitant aperiodic noise. a vowel as a relatively "pure" note or tone is produced by resonators linked to a vibratory source of this kind. the resonators amplify harmonics set up by the glottal note which fall within their bandwidth and four clear resonance bars (formants) can be seen in wide-band spectrograms in the normal voice (wide normal i 3 to 4 in the articulation of the first vowel of parents). to the extent that irregularities develop in time (frequency) and amplitude in the vibratory pattern of the vocal folds, the ear identifies harshness or roughness. a variation of frequency of as little as 1 cps can give rise to perceived roughness.24 changes in the harmonic structure give rise to perceived differences in pitch (intonation). in the narrow-band spectrogram normal ii11 a to i, a higher pitch corresponding to roughly 160 cps falls to a lower pitch of 123 cps at 21 a to p. a resonating system can, however, also function by receiving impulses in the form of sharp raps or taps. the forefinger flicking the throat just above the superior edge of the right lamina of the thyroid produces a spectrogram such as that labelled finger-flicks. each rap sets up a noise burst of very brief duration with aperiodic noise properties in which energy is distributed over the 8 khz. visible on the spectrogram. there is no inherent harmonic structure in the noise burst and even a rapid succession of such raps at rates of over 100 per second does not set up a harmonic structure. there is, therefore, no possibility of frequency modulation which would be perceived as pitch variation or intonation, even if the rate of rapping is varied significantly.* the vocal tract functions as a resonating system in response to the "rapping" input by amplifying frequencies in the noise bursts falling within the bandwidth of the resonators. the energy at such amplified frequencies decays relatively slowly and the emergent formants can be discerned at finger-flicks 1 and 2 a and b as horizontally extended smudges. a third driving force for exciting resonators is continuous turbulence causing * our discussion implicitly rejects s c r i p t u r e ' s 1 9 ( 1 9 0 6 ) t h e o r y that no overt o n e s emanate from glottal vibrations, o n l y air puffs w h i c h cause the resonators t o sound w i t h their o w n frequencies. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 88 w.a. kerr and l.w. lanham ϊ kha i b f f l j p . .̂l· . . . . h . , ; , , 16 1 7 i .1 i j 1 1 ί γΐ ll " i 1 •> ι < · ' 1 ' i 1 s > > < 1 15 «>· i k t j ι '* • o'h make her a home case ci i esophageal (narrow) case ci 11 esophageal (wide) journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) anatomical and spectrographic analysis of the voice in disease 89 friction of relatively high amplitude emanating from a point of narrow constriction at which air escapes without any interruption of the air flow. whispering is an excellent example of resonators functioning with this type of input. the point of constriction is a small v-shaped opening at the posterior end of the glottis and the vocal folds do not vibrate. whisper i (wide-band, and ii (narrow-band) show strong, transient, aperiodic noise components over the whole of the visible frequency scale and strong amplification of those frequencies falling in the bandwidths of resonators, in particular; see formant 2 in whisper ii 11 to 14 c and the corresponding area in the narrow-band display of whisper ii. this is spectrographic evidence of the discernible quality differences of whispered vowels. continuous turbulence is, for the human body as a sound producer, highly uneconomical in rapidly draining the air reservoir. for esophageal speech with its small air reservoir it is impracticable. one reason for discussing whisper here is that esophageal "burping" (see below) with a high level of concomitant friction, is seemingly far more effective than esophageal "rapping" with little friction. a point of interest connected with whisper is that, although totally lacking in harmonic structure (see whisper ii), an auditory impression of pitch variation can be created by so altering the constriction at source that the energy is differently distributed in the frequency scale. the difference between "lowpitch whisper" and "high-pitch whisper" in whisper i and ii (representing a conscious effort by the normal voice recorded on these spectrograms) is that formant i (at level a/b) and formant 2 (at level c) have, respectively, less and more energy concentrated at these relatively low frequencies. formant 3 (at e/f, i.e. 2.5 khz) shows the approximate point where the low-high energy distribution becomes inverted. in esophageal "voice" air flow from a reservoir in the upper esophagus is interrupted by crude vibrations of the esophageal sphincter which alternatively opens and closes the esophageal lumen. in esophageal sphincter vibration the closure of the valve-like exit to the air reservoir is probably brought about by muscle tension after the opening caused by air pressure. if this is the case then this vibration is not a consequence of the bernoulli effect. each opening releases a burst of noise into the resonating system at irregular, variable rates. muscle tension and air pressure would regulate the rate of vibration. case β illustrates how close this type of vibration can come to "rapping" or "tapping"; compare case bi 1 to 3 with finger-flicks. here rapping is seen to be irregular in time with an average rate of roughly 42 per second. case ci 8 to 14 shows a somewhat more rapid, regular rate of rapping at approximately 44 per second. the corresponding sections on the narrow-band spectrograms bii, cii show a total absence of harmonic structure and the formants appear as smudges darkened in the vertical bars of the raps. spectrographic evidence attests to the absence of frequency modulation in esophageal voice. a significant variation in esophageal sphincter vibration begins to show in comparing bi and ii with ci and ii (particularly 1 to 4) and is clearest when ei enters the comparison. the actual noise bursts recede in prominence and continuing aperiodic noise components over wide bandwidths are the main input to the resonators. the raps apparently smooth out into a succession of air puffs somewhat more rapid and regular, and lower in amplitude (hereafter tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 90 w.a. kerr and l.w. lanham case civ esophageal' (narrow) journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) anatomical and spectrograph analysis of the voice in disease 91 referred to as "burping" in contradistinction to "rapping"). the distinction between rapping and burping is significant because the voice of case β is very "croaky" and of low intelligibility; that of case c is unpleasantly croaky, but of high intelligibility; that of ε not at all croaky, breathy, but pleasant, and highly intelligible. although our postulation as to the site of constriction which produces random aperiodic noise components is unconfirmed experimentally, we suggest that friction noise emanates from the esophageal sphincter where the manner in which the air flow is interrupted ranges over vibratory cycles with tight closure, high esophageal pressure and "clean" break on opening (rapping), to burping with weaker, probably incomplete, closure and considerable concomitant friction. a third possible state is a still weaker interruption with a more or less open lumen allowing a continuous air flow. this would seem to be the nature of the esophageal air mechanism in oral fricatives such as [s]. if turbulence is not a consequence of esophageal vibration then pharyngeal constriction is the next most likely source. pharyngeal pressure measurements show that case d uses this mechanism and provides evidence of a vibratory mechanism involving the pharynx. we are doubtful, however, whether case c has pharyngeal constriction. case c burps without embarrassment and makes no attempt to cover the esophageal voice by pharyngeal constriction. compare spoken and sung make in ci, ii at 5 to 6 and ciii, iv at 5 to 8. the prominence of the raps recedes as the vibrations speed up* and the quantity of random, aperiodic noise is greater. in case ε the significant absence of strong noise bursts in ei is worth investigating. two significant dimensions of esophageal voice emerge from this discussion: (a) the manner in which the esophageal sphincter controls the release of air; (b) the presence and nature of pharyngeal constriction. in classifying cases of the types dealt with here it is useful to distinguish the three types of source input to the resonating system: type x pressure pulses with a harmonic structure and true frequency'modulation (friction noise may be present, but is a minor contributor). type y vibrations of low frequency in the form of noise bursts lacking a harmonic structure (rapping and burping). type ζ continuous, relatively uninterrupted friction noise. none of our cases is classified as z, but experiments with a normal vocal tract show that pharyngeal constriction could set up a "voice" of this kind. obviously there is overlap within this categorisation. ζ can clearly overlap with x and y;x and y are, as a rule, mutually exclusive. the phonation of a normal voice is only type x; a succession of glottal stops or catches cannot be produced at a rate fast enough to provide any real semblance of type y. ventricular phonation in the one case discussed below, is * t h e superior-inferior branches of tne laryngea. nerve seem t o c o n v e y the same instruction t o the cricopharyngeus muscle as it did t o muscles of the larynx. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 92 w.a. kerr and l.w. lanham 'incase 01v esophageal (narrow) λ ί » — λ , age case bv "esophageal («μδίγ s3 ϊ « λ cu < s3 60 μ s3 .—ι s μ c ρ c 2 c "" "ci c i ό <υ -ο £ £ « ω· ω μ i n u o < ω e oo α> λ cu ο ο v5 00 ~ s3 ρ c -s α» — ο £ c -o ε v5 ζ 'ξ. "λ cu ο η ο ο ο -c ο &0 on ο efi ο on e£ (n 6? ο 6? 00 6? 6? to 6? γλ •" •«a·" oo" •.ο to on on" (n r-" to ct •«a-" ô" to titiγλ •«ato to to * * * * vo, δ? γλ efi es es r ^ γλ oo γλ « 6? γλ to to to oo" ct ô" {n on" •"a-" γλ css « e£ « « ^ •«a* γλ t-^ {n γλ c-γ c-t ô" o" to on 00 on on on on .on on on oo ο ο •«a00 on (n •«ato (n 00 γλ γλ · . 1 1 in — 1 journal of the south african speech and hearing association vol. 21, december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a review of speech therapy services in ten cerebral palsy schools 49 aphasia, is not defined in the act, but eisenson's3 recent definition probably covers the children in this category at the various cerebral palsy schools. we shall be using the term developmental aphasia to refer to the impairment for a child to acquire symbols for a language system. the impairment must be of sufficient degree to interfere with the child's ability to communicate. the use of the term developmental aphasia, or one of its synonyms, implies that the child's perceptual abilities for auditory (speech) events underlies this impairment for the acquisition of auditory symbols. his expressive disturbances are a manifestation of his intake or decoding impairment. a child cannot produce language if he cannot decode the speech to which he is exposed, or if the speech remains for him sounds without sense. the definition of minimal brain dysfunction was that formulated by professor c.h. de c. murray and his committee of inquiry into the education of children with minimal brain dysfunction:9 children with minimal brain dysfunction have average or above average intellectual ability, and the motor function, vision, hearing and emotional adjustment are adequate, but they manifest specific learning disabilities or behavioural disabilities which are associated with deviations of the functioning of the central nervous system. dysfunction of the central nervous system manifests itself in different ways and in various combinations of the deviations mentioned below: impairment namely of perception, conceptualisation, language, memory, control of attention, impulse and motor function. in a recent survey murray7 found that in practice all the cerebral palsy schools are admitting children with minimal brain dysfunction and subnormal intelligence in addition to those with average and above average intelligence. the unclassified group included children with one of the undermentioned diagnosis:spinabifida, lymphangioma, pituitary dwarfism, tumour, progressive cerebrocerebellar deterioration, cerebral damage and microcephaly, freidrich's ataxia, muscular dystrophy, hydrocephalus, epilepsy, crit-du-chat syndrome, congenital developmental anomalies, lesch-niehan syndrome, awaiting diagnoses. table ii shows a comparison between the number of pupils and number of speech therapists working at each school; the number and percentage of children with speech, voice, hearing and language problems in each school; the percentage of children receiving speech therapy; the percentage of children not receiving speech therapy in each school because of a shortage of speech therapists, and the percentage of speech, voice, hearing and language problems in each diagnostic category in each school. eight hundred and fifty-one, or 56,2% of the one thousand, five hundred and fourteen children enrolled at the ten cerebral palsy schools at the time of this survey had speech, voice, hearing or language problems. 72,26% of the 851 tydskrif van die suid-afriaanse vereniging vir spraak engehoorheekunde, vol. 21, deseber 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 audrey shavell ° 0 · ° ' 0) u w li/ — u <° u £ £ — a> c 2 c a> o, α) α,-τ α 3 s « ο < ο a, o l i o π u c q « ο c . c a> c ra _ c a> ο q, a> α — x> a) ~ ν ι». ε a> π ίο. o a , u o . o ξ ό c — u £ zv a> 0ο ' co υ 0) ' ο ω a 0 α> c/j ο c α 3 u-c « ό ο h 2 -ο = >> £ = ·> s: α 5 γ · υ to ~ r « 0> 0> i^ ^ υ aj u 0) t̂ 0) q, x: α. o qi co η 5 3 & e ; · : » ι · ο. ο 5 a. s.s z u s c/3 0. _ £ cj ο ο a η w "o ο s υ co u o o o o o o ^ t n o t l/ί so 00 no.— ο ο " " > ^bx χ s " γ-) ol ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο as « bs as as as as a? as as as on νο cn •sf ο on οι 00 00 no_ r-ρ . no. on 00_ vq οο" ̂ f" oo" ̂ r" — * ο" ι/ί <ν" γ-)" no" νο it) it) γit) <-<-00 un 6s 6s 6s as as ar as as as as a? on it) it) ο on οι it) ο on '—ι —, 00„ ι/ι 00, ο . (n ο" vo oo" o" oo" tn γ-" οο" γ-)" ο" no it) γ-) it) cn γno ι/ί it) 6s as as as as a« as as no ο on 00 <ν r-00 ο on ο . (ν ζ os j ε ! γ — ο τ ι ο ^ ο — ο ο ο ο π α m u ^ t ( n ( n o o o o o o o ω ε * ^ ^ ο ο ο ^ ο ο ο ο ιε s s s ! o h £ o o o s o o — — — — ο — (ν * — — c ε ι> ο s 1η γλ ν ο ο ο ^ ^ ο ο ' ί ο ο ο (ν μ <ν <ν <υ c «> c 1) ω ~ > ω c3 ^ t5j ι. η η α o o n k " > ( n r o t < x > o o r υ-> μ π (ν tj· γ** j s s s : ο lα) u. η α u. η α ο <ν ο ί!7ι ο ο ο •ο c ε ύ .2 α> μ •ο c ε -η l i t t. e λ -ϊ u η ρ α» α» t-c υ α» 3 u α £ ζ βί m (ο ο ί ν ο ο ^ ο ο (χ) —ι — ο ο̂ "3" "3" (ν r<χ> — — γ*-> — ( χ ) γ 0 γ ( ν ο ( ν γ 0 0 — 00γ-ι/->"3-(ν<χ><χ> — — c! = ξ > > > = ξ > < χ r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) areview of speech therapy services in ten cerebral palsy schools 53 i a s s e s s m e n t of patients in the ten c e r e b r a l p a l s y schools nine of the ten schools used a battery of tests to assess speech, hearing and language abilities of their patients. five of the ten schools did not include details of hearing tests in their replies but almost certainly have some system of hearing screening and/or free field testing and/or formal audiometric assessment. table iv gives details of the types of tests used. s c h o o l hearing a r t i c u l a t i o n language r e a d i n g &, o t h e r t e s t s 1 free field hearing p h o n e t i c illinois test of psychodetailed examitests i n v e n t o r y linguistic a b i l i t i e s n a t i o n of s p e e c h filtered music a f r i k a a n s p e a b o d y p i c t u r e vom e c h a n i s m , hearing tests & english c a b u l a r y test b r e a t h i n g , basic pure t o n e a u d i o r e n f r e w n o r t h w e s t e r n s y n t a x f u n c t i o n s &, m e t r y articulas c r e e n i n g test p h o n a t i o n . s p e e c h a u d i o tion r e e l scale assessing m e t r y a t t a i n l a n g u a g e skills in w e p m a n a u d i t o r y m e n t infancy p e r c e p t i o n test h o u s t o n test for lang o l d m a n f r i s t o e guage d e v e l o p m e n t • w o o d c o c k audir e y n e l l d e v e l o p m e n t a l t o r y p e r c e p t i o n l a n g u a g e scales t e s t r e n f r e w w o r d f i n d i n g r o y a l n a t i o n a l v o c a b u l a r y scale i n s t i t u t e for t h e r e n f r e w f l o y d a c t i o n deaf p i c t u r e p i c t u r e test screening test v o c a b u l a r y test for of hearing y o u n g c h i l d r e n (modified from w a t t s ) r e n f r e w bus s t o r y e i s e n s o n e x a m i n i n g for a p h a s i a & related d i s t u r b a n c e s sklar a p h a s i a scale 11 f r e e field g o l d m a n illinois test of p s y c h o g a t e s r e a d i n g h e a r i n g tests f r i s t o e linguistic a b i l i t i e s r e a d i n e s s pitch p i p e s a r t i c u l a p e a b o d y p i c t u r e von e a l e s a n a l y s i s t i o n t e s t c a b u l a r y t e s t basic c o n c e p t i n v e n t o r y m c d o n a l d , c h a n c e l a n g u a g e e v a l u a t i o n s c h u e l l a p h a s i a d e t a i l e d e x a m i n a t i o n of s p e e c h m e c h a n i s m , b r e a t h i n g , basic f u n c t i o n s & p h o n a t i o n iii w e p m a n a u d i r e n f r e w illinois test of p s y c h o u . c . t . s c h o l a s t i c t o r y p e r c e p t i o n a r t i c u l a linguistic a b i l i t i e s t e s t s t y c a r test of t i o n r e y n e l l d e v e l o p m e n t a l d e t a i l e d e x a m i hearing a t t a i n l a n g . scales n a t i o n of s p e e c h m e n t r e n f r e w f l o y d a c t i o n m e c h a n i s m , t e s t p i c t u r e test v o c a b u l a r y t e s t for y o u n g c h i l d r e n r e n f r e w bus s t o r y r e n f r e w word f i n d i n g v o c a b . scale n o r t h w e s t e r n s y n t a x b r e a t h i n g , basic f u n c t i o n s & p h o n a t i o n table iv continued tydskrif van die suid-afriaanse vereniging vir spraak engehoorheekunde, vol. 21, deseber 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 i audrey shavell s c h o o l hearing a r t i c u l a t i o n l a n g u a g e r e a d i n g & o t h e r t e s t s s c r e e n i n g test e i s e n s o n e x a m i n i n g for a p h a s i a a n d related d i s t u r b a n c e s a b b r e v i a t e d schuell a p h a s i a test m y k l e b u s t p e a b o d y p i c t u r e voc a b u l a r y test h u t t o n c u r r i e semi d i a g n o s t i c test iv free field h e a r i n g t e s t s pitch pipes p u r e t o n e a u d i o m e t r y s p e e c h a u d i o m e t r y p h o n e t i c i n v e n t o r y illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities . v w e p m a n a u d i t o r y p e r c e p t i o n test p e a b o d y p i c t u r e voc a b . test illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities vi i n f o r m a l h e a r i n g t e s t s w e p m a n a u d i t o r y p e r c e p t i o n test p h o n e t i c i n v e n t o r y a f r i k a a n s e a r t i k u l a s i e o n d e r s o e k illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities p e a b o d y p i c t u r e voc a b u l a r y test v e r b a l l a n g u a g e dev e l o p m e n t scale basic c o n c e p t invent o r y p r e s c h o o l language d e v e l o p m e n t scale t e a c h e r s a u d i t o r y & l a n g u a g e kit d e t a i l e d e x a m i n a t i o n of s p e e c h m e c h a n i s m , b r e a t h i n g , basic f u n c t i o n s & p h o n a t i o n vii illinois t e s t of p s y c h o linguistic a b i l i t i e s viii a u d i o m e t r i c t e s t s a r t i c u l a t i o n t e s t s a p h a s i a t e s t s ' ix g o l d m a n f r i s t o e w o o d c o c k a u d i t o r y discrimin a t i o n p h o n e t i c i n v e n t o r y p e a b o d y p i c t u r e voc a b u l a r y test illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities n a t i o n a l b u r e a u of e d u c a t i o n a l & social r e s e a r c h r e a d i n g & lang u a g e t e s t s / x a f r i k a a n s e a r t i k u l a s i e o n d e r s o e k p e a b o d y p i c t u r e voc a b u l a r y test illinois t e s t of p s y c h o linguistic a b i l i t i e s r e n f r e w l a n g u a g e scales a n a l y s i s of s p e e c h s a m p l e s of c h i l d r e n neales a n a l y s i s of r e a d i n g a b i l i t y table iv. tests used to diagnose hearing, articulation, language and reading in cerebral palsy schools. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 21, december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a review of speech therapy services in ten cerebral palsy schools 55 reference to table iv indicates the increasing need felt by speech therapists working at cerebral palsy schools to assess their patients as objectively, scientifically and comprehensively as possible. the multifaceted problems presented by patients at a cerebral palsy school are of a very complex nature and without utilizing objective testing procedures one could easily underestimate the severity of a language disorder and/or auditory perception problem while one deals with the inhibition of pathological reflex patterns and facilitation of more normal movements and vice-versa. no single patient is given all the tests, but it is important for the speech therapist to have a wide enough battery of tests to adequately assess all aspects of speech and language. filtered music hearing testing has been successfully used as a hearing screening technique at school i for the past year. the use of this method enables one to test the hearing of very young and/or retarded and/or hyperactive and/or unco-operative c h i l d r e n 1 3 . the v a l u e of j o i n t a s s e s s m e n t of patients by physiotherapist, o c c u p a t i o n a l t h e r a p i s t a n d speech t h e r a p i s t speech therapists working at eight of the ten schools in this survey felt there was real merit in joint assessment of the patients. this is particularly valuable when dealing with the severely handicapped cerebral palsied patient. in the writer's opinion it is one of the best methods of 'in-service training' for therapists and teaches the speech therapist to see her patient as a 'whole' — the essential basis of all the modern techniques of t r e a t m e n t . 2 , 1 0 , n · 1 2 case c o n f e r e n c e s regular case conferences are held at seven of the ten schools at varying intervals ranging from every six months to every twenty-four months depending on the enrolment at the various schools. speech therapists in all these schools found the regular case conference a most valuable opportunity for an interchange of ideas about the patient. problems and difficulties being experienced by one member of the team can be discussed and a combined programme devised to overcome or reduce them. since the majority of our patients attend the cerebral palsy.schools in south africa from infancy to late adolescence, we have to live with our failures as well as our successes and it is at the case conference that one sees the integration of the various therapeutic, educational, psychological and social services. as the children mature, their needs change and the emphasis often alters from intensive therapy and formal education to reduced 'maintenance therapy' and pre-vocational training. the value of the case conference is enhanced if all members of the team know in advance which patients are to be discussed each week, so that they can adequately prepare formal reports including the most recent test results in each department. as assessment should ideally be an 'on-going process' the case conference provides the ideal opportunity to obtain the latest information on the patient from every aspect. this in turn leads to a deeper understanding and appreciation of the patient's problems and difficulties. tydskrif van die suid-afriaanse vereniging vir spraak engehoorheeikunde, vol. 21, deseber 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 audrey shavell the u s e of communication b o a r d s or other n o n v e r b a l systems o f communication this question was included because it is felt that therapists are becoming more realistic. as our experience in this field increases we appreciate that some severely handicapped cerebral palsied patients will never communicate 'easily' on a verbal level and the effort of vocalization and oral communication frequently triggers off such severe spasm or such fluctuations of muscle tone that the patient is rendered completely 'speechless' and very frustrated'. by providing an alternate non-verbal means of communication early in the therapy programme we can break this vicious circle and reduce the pressure for verbalization and frustration at not being able to indicate simple everyday wants and needs. mcdonald and schultz 6 lucidly explain how the early use of communication boards for severely handicapped children, facilitates the development of inner language and the desire to communicate. hagen, porter and brink 4 reported on the use of a non-verbal electro-mechanical communication device for retarded children with cerebral palsy. five of the ten schools in the present survey were using or had used a non-verbal communication system. details will be found in table v. s c h o o l t y p e of n o n v e r b a l c o m m u n i c a t i o n s y s t e m used i p i c t u r e , p i c t u r e a n d w o r d , w o r d s o n l y , m o u n t e d o n c a r d b o a r d o r hardb o a r d covered in t r a n s p a r e n t p lastic. p a t i e n t uses h e a d , arm or foot t o p o i n t a n d i n d i c a t e n e e d s a n d c a n f o r m u l a t e s e n t e n c e s . ii m a g n e t i c p i c t u r e c a r d o n wheel chair iii p r e v i o u s l y t r i e d c o m m u n i c a t i o n b o a r d . n o w e x p e r i m e n t i n g w i t h p a g e t ' s g e s t u r e language s y s t e m . iv t y p i n g used a s a n a l t e r n a t e m o d e o f c o m m u n i c a t i o n . vi p i c t u r e b l o c k b o a r d ; m o v a b l e p i c t u r e c a r d b o a r d o n " c o u n t i n g f r a m e " p r i n c i p l e a t t a c h e d t o w h e e l ch air. table v. description of non-verbal communication systems used in cerebral palsy schools. / / / t r a i n i n g in h a n d l i n g the n e u r o l o g i c a l l y impaired patient work with the neurologically impaired patient is highly specialized and one of the questions asked the therapists was whether they had received special training in this field on (a) an under-graduate level (b) post-graduate level. their replies are analysed in table vi. at the present time the only post-graduate course available in south africa is the diploma for therapists in special education (cerebral palsied) at the journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 21, december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a review of speech therapy services in ten cerebral palsy schools university of south africa. this course does not include actual principles or techniques of treatment. five of the twenty-six speech therapists in the present survey had completed a bobath course in the neurophysiological treatment of cerebral palsy either in london or when dr and mrs bobath visited south africa or with a 'bobath trained' teacher. two therapists had attended professor margaret rood's course on sensory stimulation techniques. the need for a theoretical and practical course on a post-graduate level for therapists dealing with neurologically handicapped patients is becoming ever more urgent. it should include a detailed study of normal development from birth to three years of age and all the neurophysiological techniques of treatment. the final question of the survey requested the speech therapists to list the topics they would like to discuss if it were possible for speech therapists working at cerebral palsy schools to meet. the forty topics listed appear in the appendix. conclusion (1) the far-sighted policy of the department of national education in allowing pre-nursery school children and young babies to receive treatment on an out-patient basis at the cerebral palsy schools in south africa is highly commendable. this policy has led to a decrease in the numbers of severely handicapped, immobile older children because starting treatment at a really young age has many advantages. some of these are: (a) the prevention of contractures and deformities or reduction in severity of contractures and deformities. (b) the inhibition of pathological reflex patterns and facilitation of normal postural reactions and movement sequences enable the children to be more mobile and independent which in turn widens their environment and favourably influences language development. (c) the prevention of the development of poor patterns of behaviour, learning and attitudes by correct 'early' handling and supportive counselling of parents of young patients. (d) achieving maximum mental growth and development. elizabeth kong states: on the whole the results have been most encouraging. furthermore, it is striking that there are fewer mentally retarded children among the cases of cerebral palsy treated early. we believe that their normal motor experience has given these children a chance to develop their normal potentials, whereas the children treated later lack this experience and do not have the same chance to develop normally, so that, unless they are of superior intelligence, they are likely to attain only a subnormal performance. in cases where treatment has not been started until the second year of life the results have not been so satisfactory.5 (2) the question of the placement of children in the minimal brain dysfunction and aphasic categories in cerebral palsy schools will have to be considered. there is no doubt that these children benefit from the educational programme and therapies but if their numbers continue to increase, tydskrif van die suid-afriaanse vereniging vir spraak engehoorheeikunde, vol. 21, deseber 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 audrey shavell school number of speech therapists number received undergraduate training in c.p. number + details of post graduate training i 6 2 2 bobath, 1 rood, 3 diploma in special education (university of south africa) ii 3 3 0 iii 3 2 1 bobath, 1 rood, 1 m.a. psychology iv 3 1 1 diploma in special education (cerebral palsy) v 1 1 0 vi 3 1 1 bobath + diploma in • special ed (hearing) 1 diploma in special •education (cerebral palsy) vii 4 2 i 1 bobath, 3 diploma in special education (cerebral palsy) viii 1 1 0 ix 1 1 0 x ' 1 1 0 totals 26 15 therapists received 1-2 . months undergraduate training 5 therapists bobath trained; 2 shortcourse in rood method; 8 therapists d.s.e. cerebral palsy; 1 therapist d.s.e. hearing. table vi. details of numbers of speech therapists at each of the ten , schools who had received underand/or post-graduate training in cerebral palsy schools. provision of separate facilities for them will have to be made. one of the ten schools in the present survey already has 54,78% of its total enrolment in the minimal brain dysfunction group. the child with a serious language impairment, associated behavioural anomalies, perseveration, disinhibition, short attention span, and a poor memory needs more than journal of the south african spch and hearing association, vol. 21, december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a review of speech therapy services in ten cerebral palsy schools 59 two or three weekly sessions of speech therapy. he or she needs an organized, 'structured' nursery school or pre-grade programme and daily speech and occupational therapy to overcome his learning, perception and language disabilities as rapidly as possible. if these children are excluded from cerebral palsy schools an alternate programme of schooling and treatment will have to be evolved for them. although they are difficult to handle especially when very young, they are among our most rewarding cases and our regret is that video-taped progress records have not been kept. if treatment of these children is started early many of them can go to normal schools in standard i or ii. (3) merely comparing the ratio of speech therapists to the number of children receiving speech therapy at any given school can lead to a false impression because the frequency of treatment is excluded. success in this work depends very largely on the intensity and frequency of treatment, repetition and reinforcement being the keys. the numbers of speech therapy posts at cerebral palsy schools should be increased so that: (a) all children who require speech therapy will receive sufficient treatment (b) there is time for "in-service" training of new staff, lectures, case demonstrations, joint 'team' assessments, case conferences and student training. the original formula for the appointment of speech therapists at cerebral palsy schools was drawn up many years ago when these schools catered . almost exclusively for the 'pure' cerebral palsied child with, a visible motor problem. the population of the cerebral palsy schools currently include as many and in one case more children with 'soft neurological' signs who have a higher incidence of language disorders and problems in conceptualization. if we compare the percentage of language disorders in the cerebral palsy group to that in the minimal brain dysfunction group it will be seen that in six out of the ten schools the percentage is significantly higher in the minimal brain dysfunction group. this difference is increased further if we add the aphasic group to the minimal brain dysfunction group. (4) speech therapists working at cerebral palsy schools should familiarise themselves with the latest tests and should critically evaluate the construction of new tests, their validity, reliability and suitability for south african children so that they select appropriate tests to pin-point the child's problems. an urgent need for afrikaans translation and standardization of language tests exists. (5) the general impression gained from the one hundred percent return of questionnaires and the detail in which most therapists completed the questionnaire, is that speech therapists working at cerebral palsy schools find this work both rewarding and challenging. although we may not see results as quickly as our colleagues in general speech therapy practice, there are many advantages to working as a member of a,dynamic team tydskrif van die suid-afriaanse vereniging vir spraak engehoorheeikunde, vol. 2 , deseber 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 audrey shavell where medical consultants assess patients together with therapists. in addition, members of the team are accessible for repeated assessments and discussions and are also receptive to suggestions. by virtue of our geographic isolation from leading centres in britain, the united states of america and europe, our.therapists have not become exponents of one method or technique but are attempting to use an holistic eclectic approach. this freedom of choice of treatment results in a very real sense of responsibility on the part of the therapist. a successful eclectic approach is possible only when the therapist has a sound knowledge of the different techniques, including appreciation of their common denominator and is therefore able to select the most suitable approach to meet the child's needs at a particular time. for this reason provision of post-graduate training in the habilitation of the neurologically impaired patient is becoming a matter of urgency. acknowledgements most sincere thanks are extended to the principals and speech therapists of the ten cerebral palsy schools who so willingly co-operated in completing the questionnaires on which this survey was based. references (1) act no. 41 of 1967 (1968): to provide for the establishment, maintenance, administration and control of, and the rendering of financial aid in respect of schools at which certain classes of education are provided for conferring upon provincial councils powers in respect of certain classes of education and for matters incidental thereto. republic of south africa government gazette, extraordinary. vol. 42, no. 2253, december. (2) bobath, b., bobath, k. (1972): the neurodevelopmental approach to treatment. in physical therapy sen'ices in the developmental disabilities. ed. paul pearson, c.c. thomas, springfield, 111. (3) eisenson, j. (\912)\ aphasia in children harper and row, n.y. (4) hagen, c., porter, w., brink, j. (1973): non-verbal communication: an alternate mode of communication for the child with severe cerebral palsy. /. speech & hearing dis., 38, 4, 448. / (5) k5ng, e. (1966): very early treatment of cerebral palsy. develop. med. child neurol., 8, 198-202. (6) mcdonald, e.t. schultz, a.r. (1973): communication boards for cerebral palsied children. j. speech & hearing dis., 38, 1, 73. (7) murray, c.h. de c. (1973): report to the management committee of the national cerebral palsy division of the national council for the care of cripples in south africa. (8) oosthuizen, i., pelser, j.w. (1973): the use of respirator in the treatment of cerebral palsied children. physiotherapy, 29, 2. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 21 december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a review of speech therapy services in ten cerebral palsy schools 61 (9) report of the committee of inquiry into the education of children with minimal brain dysfunction. (no. 72/1969), government printer, pretoria. (10) rood, m. (1969): the use of sensory receptors to activate, facilitate and inhibit motor response, autonomic and somatic changes in developmental sequence. unpublished notes, university of southern california and instructional course, south africa. (11) vojta, v. (1973): early treatment of children at risk for cerebral palsy. analysis of end results. monatsschrift fur kinderheilkunde, 121, 271. (12) voss, d.e. (1972): proprioceptive neuromuscular facilitation: the p.n.f. method. chapter 5 in physical therapy in the developmental disabilities, pearson, p. ed., c. c. thomas, springfield, 111. (13) waldman, h.l., cocroft, c.l., ludi, b. (1974): filtered music: a hearing test for young children. sa medical journal, 48, 1772. appendix topics for discussion suggested by speech therapists at the cerebral palsy schools (1) the basis for selecting cases for speech therapy at cerebral palsy schools (2) when should speech therapy be terminated? (3) the frequency of speech therapy especially for aphasic patients (4) improved programming and methods of presentation and work for children with congenital auditory imperception (5) views and methods employed by therapists with language cases (6) later incidence of and type of language problems in children who initially had delayed speech (7) similarities and differences in symptomatology and treatment of receptive aphasia and the hard of hearing child (8) the concept of childhood aphasia (9) techniques for activating a paralyzed velum in cases of upper motor neurone lesions (10) how to improve breathing for the cerebral palsied child (11) the diagnosis of aphasia in the cerebral palsied child (12) intellectual capacity or level and speech therapy. should speech therapy be provided for children with low i.q. levels? (13) positioning and techniques to facilitate mouth movements and techniques of inhibition of pathological reflexes tydskrif v die suid-afriaanse vereniging vir spraak engehoorheeikunde, vol. 21, deseniber 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 audrey shavell (14) drooling (15) specific hearing tests (16) devising and instituting home programmes (17) handling the infant cerebral palsied child and his mother (18) ideas on running parent counselling groups (19) the role of the social worker in the cerebral palsy school (20) use of non-verbal communication in cerebral palsy. (21) the.speech therapist's role in remedial reading & spelling training of brain injured children with poor language.'should we treat dyslexics in cerebral palsy schools? (22) diagnostic procedures (23) treatment of the athetoid child (24) standardization of language and auditory discrimination tests on south african children (25) translation and standardization of language and other tests into afrikaans (26) tests and training programmes to improve afrikaans language problems ^ e.g. generative grammar (27) feeding the cerebral palsied child (28) orientation of treatment of severe cerebral palsy cases who will never talk (29) motivating the cerebral palsied child and motivating parents to develop independence (30) is under-graduate training in cerebral palsy satisfactory? (31) language therapy vs. speech therapy which one should enjoy preference? (32) recent research in speech therapy techniques for the cerebral palsied child (33) how professional are we? (34) group opinion on our medical orientation (35) exchange of ideas on therapy approaches with the cerebral palsied child (36) discussion on newest developments in the cerebral palsied field medical and therapeutic (37) minimal cerebral dysfunction in the cerebral palsy schools evalua' tion and handling (38) compiling and carrying out an integrated therapy and educational programme (39) record keeping, test results, doctors reports etc. (40) experimental work execution and results journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 21, december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) audiometry weber and rinne tests as compared to pure-tone thresholds • thompson, alison κ., b.a. (sp. & η. th.) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) speech therapy department, general hospital, johannesburg summary results of the audiometric weber and rinne tests were compared t o pure t o n e thresholds in 1 8 5 bantu patients. the frequency of 1 0 0 0 hz was selected as being most suitable for weber and rinne testing. t h e weber w a s found t o be of limited diagnostic value even w i t h unilateral c o n d u c t i v e losses whilst the rinne displays a fair degree of efficiency and is of value as a routine supplement t o audiometric threshold tests. opsomming resultate van die o u d i o m e t r i e s e weber en rinne t o e t s e is m e t s u i w e r t o o n drempels vergelyk. 1 8 5 b a n t o e pasiente is as proefpersone gebruik. die frekwensie van looohz is as die geskikste vir die weber en rinne t o e t s e gereken. die weber is, selfs met eensydige geleidings verlies, van beperkte diagnostiese waarde gevind, terwyl die rinne 'n redelike graad van doeltreffendheid g e t o o n het en aanvullend b y die o u d i o m e t r i e s e drempel t o e t s e gebruik kan word. at the hearing clinic at baragwanath hospital, johannesburg, routine audiometric weber and'rinne tests were fairly frequently found to be inconsistent with pure tone thresholds. in the present study, a large number of pure tone audiograms were compared with audiometric rinne and weber results. in recent years, very little has appeared in the literature concerning the weber and rinne tests. that which has appeared deals almost exclusively with testing by means of tuning forks. most authors stress the need to include the weber and rinne in a battery of tuning fork tests including the schwabach and gelle t e s t s . 1 ' 3 , 6 , 8 , 1 1 , 1 ? reliability of these tests has been questioned by these workers but they are nevertheless considered to be useful in supplementing audiometric results. testing by means of the audiometric bone vibrator has certain advantages over testing by means of tuning f o r k s . 2 , 8 , 1 3 the bone vibrator maintains its , intensity output at any desired level, whereas the tuning fork fades rapidly in intensity, especially in the high frequencies. 1 1 in addition, the bone vibrator permits a standard presentation which is independent of the ear of the o p e r a t o r . 5 , 1 3 materials and methods hearing assessments of 185 south african bantu of both sexes with an age range of 12 to 75 years were analysed. these were taken randomly from the tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheeikunde, vol'. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the breckwoldt laryngo-reflectoscope by dr. g. h. breckwoldt a s s o c i a t e p r o f e s s o r of s p e e c h , s t . l o u i s u n i v e r s i t y , m i s s o u r i , u . s . a . introduction a l t h o u g h t h e b r e c k w o l d t l a r y n g o r e f l e c t o s c o p e * s h o u l d p r o v e a u s e f u l i n s t r u m e n t i n t h e c o n s u l t i n g r o o m of t h e m e d i c a l d o c t o r p a r t i c u l a r l y t h e e . n . t . s p e c i a l i s t t h e a u t h o r d e s i g n e d i t e s p e c i a l l y for t h e s p e e c h t h r a p i s t . t h e l a t t e r w o u l d n e v e r a t t e m p t t o t r e a t a n a r t i c u l a t o r y d i s o r d e r w i t h o u t t h o r o u g h l y e x a m i n i n g t h e i n d i v i d u a l ' s s p e e c h m e c h a n i s m . w h y s h o u l d h e e v e r v e n t u r e o r b e f o r c e d t o h a n d l e a v o i c e d i s o r d e r w i t h o u t h a v i n g h a d a s m u c h a s a l o o k a t t h e i n d i v i d u a l ' s v o i c e m e c h a n i s m ? a v o i c e d i s o r d e r c a n n e v e r b e j u d g e d b y e a r a l o n e ; i t m u s t b e e x a m i n e d v i s u a l l y . t h e t h e r a p i s t h a s t o l e a r n h o w t o e x a m i n e t h e l a r y n x of t h e v o i c e d e f e c t i v e a s w e l l a s h e l e a r n s t o e x a m i n e t h e facial, o r a l , p h a r y n g e a l , d e n t a l , e t c . , c o n d i t i o n of t h e s p e e c h d e f e c t i v e . a f t e r h a v i n g s t u d i e d t h e a n a t o m y , p h y s i o l o g y , n e u r o l o g y a n d p a t h o l o g y of t h e v o i c e m e c h a n i s m , h e s h o u l d b e t r a i n e d i n a v o i c e l a b o r a t o r y t o g a i n p r a c t i c a l e x p e r i e n c e i n t h e t e c h n i q u e s of v o i c e e x a m i n a t i o n , w i t h o u t w h i c h a n y k i n d of t h e r a p y ( u n l e s s g i v e n u n d e r t h e d i r e c t a n d c o n s t a n t s u p e r v i s i o n of a m e d i c a l m a n ) m i g h t b e d a n g e r o u s . o n c e t h e s p e e c h t h e r a p i s t k n o w s n o t o n l y h o w t o l o o k a t a l a r y n x , b u t a l s o w h a t i n d e t a i l t o l o o k for, h e w i l l b e a g r e a t a s s e t t o h i s p r o f e s s i o n . c a s e s w h o c o m e t o t h e t h e r a p i s t a n d w h o s e v o i c e e x a m i n a t i o n s h o w s o r g a n i c d e f e c t s w i l l b e r e f e r r e d t o a m e d i c a l d o c t o r o n t h e o t h e r h a n d t h e m e d i c a l m a n w i l l b e a b l e t o s e n d t o t h e v o i c e l a b o r a t o r y * the author could have coined a homogeneous greek word like "antilamposcope" (from antilampo": to reflect light) but preferred to give the instrument the mnemonically more suitable graecolatin name. t h o s e v o i c e c a s e s w h o d o n o t r e q u i r e m e d i c a l t r e a t m e n t , b u t a r e i n n e e d of v o i c e t h e r a p y . t h i s c o o p e r a t i o n b e t w e e n t h e v o i c e l a b o r a t o r y a n d t h e m e d i c a l p r o f e s s i o n w i l l e n s u r e t h a t v o i c e c a s e s w i l l r e c e i v e t h e b e s t p o s s i b l e c a r e . 1. history of indirect laryngoscopy t h e f i r s t s u c c e s s f u l l a r y n g o s c o p i c o b s e r v a t i o n k n o w n w a s n o t t h e e x a m i n a t i o n of a p a t i e n t o r s u b j e c t b y a d o c t o r , b u t a n a u t o l a r y n g o s c o p y p e r f o r m e d , i n 1854, b y a n a r t i s t . i t w a s t h e f a m o u s s p a n i s h s i n g e r a n d t e a c h e r of s i n g i n g m a n u e l g a r c i a . h e w a s c u r i o u s a b o u t t h e f u n c t i o n i n g of h i s l a r y n x , a n d u s i n g a d e n t a l m i r r o r for o b s e r v i n g , h e w a s t h e f i r s t p e r s o n t o s e e t h e v o c a l folds of a l i v i n g h u m a n b e i n g . i n 1857, t h e p h y s i c i a n c z e r m a k h a d t h e i d e a of p e r f o r m i n g g a r c i a ' s l a r y n x e x a m i n a t i o n w i t h a n a r t i f i c i a l l i g h t . i n 1858 h e p u b l i s h e d h i s l a r y n g o s c o p i c f i n d i n g s ( i n s i t z u n g s b e r i c h t , m a t h . n a t . w i s s . a b t . , xxix» 5 5 7 · cf. p a n c o n c e l l i c a l z i a : " q u e l l e n a t l a s , h a m b u r g , 1940, p . 3 0 ) a n d a p l a t e w h i c h illus t r a t e s a n a u t o l a r y n g o s c o p y , t h e f i r s t p i c t u r e e v e r p u b l i s h e d of t h i s t y p e of e x a m i n a t i o n . t w o y e a r s l a t e r , i n i 8 6 0 , c z e r m a k p u b l i s h e d h i s b o o k " d e r k e h l k o p f s p i e g e l u n d s e i n e v e r w e r t u n g fur p h y s i o l o g i e u n d m e d i z i n " l e i p z i g , (cf. p . c a l z i a : q u e l l e n a t l a s , p . 3 0 ) , i n w h i c h w e find t h e f i r s t i l l u s t r a t i o n of a ' l a r y n g o s c o p y p e r f o r m e d o n a p a t i e n t . s i n c e t h e n , e x c e p t for c h a n g e s i n m a t e r i a l a n d q u a l i t y of t h e l a r y n x m i r r o r , h e a d m i r r o r a n d l i g h t , t h e t e c h n i q u e of i n d i r e c t a u t o a n d h e t e r o l a r y n g o s c o p y h a s r e m a i n e d t h e s a m e . t h e o r i g i n a l i t y of t h e b r e c k w o l d l a r y n g o r e f l e c t o s c o p e , d e s i g n e d b y t h e a u t h o r i n 1962 l i e s i n t h e i n t r o d u c t i o n of a t r a n s p a r e n t m i r r o r w h i c h e n a b l e s b o t h t h e e x a m i n e r a n d t h e e x a m i n e e t o h a v e a n e f f o r t l e s s 0 journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a n d p e r f e c t v i e w of t h e l a r y n x . t h e i n s t r u m e n t c o m b i n e s all a d v a n t a g e s of a u t o a n d h e t e r o l a r y n g o s c o p y a n d h a s t h e r e f o r e m u l t i p l e u s e s . i t c a n b e o p e r a t e d w i t h a c o n t i n u o u s o r a s t r o b o s c o p i c l i g h t s o u r c e . 2. description of the laryngoreflectoscope t h e d i m e n s i o n s of t h e i n s t r u m e n t a r e m a d e t o f a c i l i t a t e t h e r e f l e c t o s c o p i c e x a m i n a t i o n w i t h t a b l e a n d c h a i r s of a v e r a g e h e i g h t , s o t h a t a p a r t f r o m a b r i g h t l a m p (_|_ 100 w a t t ) * o r a l a r y n g o s t r o b o s c o p i c l a m p * * n o f u r t h e r e q u i p m e n t is r e q u i r e d . t h e i n s t r u m e n t (cf. f i g 1) c o n s i s t s of:(1) a r e f l e c t o r ( 3 ^ " d i a m . ) , of t h e t y p e of a m e d i c a l h e a d m i r r o r , t h e c e n t r a l v i e w i n g h o l e of w h i c h is b l o c k e d u p ; (2) a p l a n e t r a n s p a r e n t mirror ( 3 " x 6 " ) , of t h e m i r r o p a n e t y p e : (3) t w o ball j o i n t s , o n e a t t a c h e d t o e a c h m i r r o r ; (4) t w o g o o s e n e c k a t t a c h m e n t s , ( e a c h 1 2 " i n l e n g t h ) , w h i c h a r e e x t r e m e l y f l e x i b l e b u t r e m a i n s t a b l e in e a c h d e s i r e d p o s i t i o n ; fig. 1 the ideal light (i.e. daylight type) is the burton lamp (n.1277), made by the burton manufacturing company, el segundo, california. with an adjustment to narrow its light beam the laryngo-synchronstroboscope ks 3 has proved ideal in conjunction with the laryngo-reflectoscope. the ks 3 instrument is made by r. timcke, hamburg, germany. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) (5) (6) (7) t w o b a l l j o i n t s , o n e a t t a c h e d t o t h e t o p of e a c h g o o s e n e c k ; t w o j o i n t c o n n e c t o r s , s e c u r i n g t h e m i r r o r s t o t h e g o o s e n e c k ; a g o o s e n e c k h o l d e r d i a m . , h i g h ) , i n t o t h e t o p of w h i c h t h e t w o g o o s e n e c k s a r e a t t a c h e d a n d t h e b o t t o m p o r t i o n of w h i c h h a s a s h o r t c o a r s e s c r e w t h r e a d p r o j e c t i o n ; (8) a riser b l o c k ( 1 " d i a m . , 3 ' h i g h ) w i t h m a l e a n d f e m a l e c o a r s e t h r e a d ( | " d i a m . ) e n d i n g s t o m a t c h t h e t h r e a d o n t h e b a s e of t h e g o o s e n e c k h o l d e r ; a n a r m ( 1 3 ^ " l o n g , 1 " w i d e , 3 / 8 " t h i c k ) , w h i c h a t o n e e n d h a s a n u n t h r e a d e d h o l e ( § " d i a m . ; t h e c e n t r e b e i n g f r o m t h e e n d p o i n t of t h e a r m ) a t t h e o t h e r e n d a t h r e a d e d h o l e ( | " d i a m . ; t h e c e n t r e b e i n g f r o m t h e e n d p o i n t of t h e a r m ) , i n t o w h i c h t h e g o o s e n e c k h o l d e r s c r e w s ; (9) ( 1 0 ) a c t y p e t a b l e c l a m p , s u i t a b l e t o c l a m p o n t a b l e t o p e d g e s of a t h i c k n e s s b e w e e n 2 ± " t o f'; ( 1 1 ) a w i n g s c r e w ( 1 " d i a m . ) f i t t i n g i n t o t h e t o p of t h e c c l a m p a n d h o r i z o n t a l l y s e c u r i n g t h e a r m t o it; (12) a laryngoscopic mirror; (13) a light source. t h e i n s t r u m e n t c a n b e b e n t t o l o w p o s i t i o n s (cf. f i g . 2) o r c a n b e e x t e n d e d b y m e a n s of t h e r i s e r b l o c k . t h e " l a r y n g o r e f l e c t o s c o p e " is s t o r e d in a n e s p e c i a l l y t a i l o r e d b o x , w h i c h k e e p s e a c h p a r t s e c u r e l y in p l a c e a n d m a k e s s h i p p i n g p o s s i b l e . fig. 2 & * *. ** <«*»*» λ \ ri-v •π8"' journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) fig. 3 august, 1963 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3. uses (a) t h e l a r y n g o r e f l e c t o s c o p e is u s e d for indirect laryngoscopy and laryngostrob o s c o p y . t h e r e f l e c t o r , r e p l a c i n g t h e h e a d b a n d a n d m i r r o r , g i v e s t h e e x a m i n e r m o r e f r e e d o m of m o v e m e n t . t h e t r a n s p a r e n t m i r r o r , a p a r t f r o m i t s r e f l e c t o s c o p i c u s e , s e r v e s a s a p r o t e c t i n g s h i e l d . (n.b.: f o r t h e e x a m i n a t i o n of i n f e c t i o n s u s p e c t c a s e s a l a r g e r g l a s s s h i e l d c a n b e m o u n t e d o n t h e i n s t r u m e n t ) . ( b ) t h e l a r y n g o r e f l e c t o s c o p e is u s e d t o instruct students of voice science and voice p a t h o l o g y . i t f a c i l i t a t e s : i. t h e f o c u s s i n g of l i g h t ( o n h i m s e l f o r a s u b j e c t ) ; ii. t h e s t u d y of h i s o w n l a r y n x (cf. f i g . 3); iii. t h e s t u d y of h i s o w n l a r y n x , w h i l e o t h e r s w a t c h ; iv. t h e e x a m i n a t i o n of a l a r y n x , w h i l e t h e e x a m i n e e h a s a s i m u l t a n e o u s l o o k ; v . t h e e x a m i n a t i o n of h i m s e l f b y a n o t h e r p e r s o n , w h i l e h e l o o k s a t h i s o w n l a r y n x a t t h e s a m e t i m e ; v i . t h e d e m o n s t r a t i o n of a s u b j e c t ' s l a r y n x , w h i l e a g r o u p of b e t w e e n 6 8 p e o p l e l o o k o n . ( c ) i t is i d e a l l y s u i t e d t o s h o w t h e l a r y n x t o certain cases for pedagogic reasons. e x a m p l e s a r e p e o p l e w h o s m o k e , d r i n k o r i n d u l g e in v o c a l a b u s e , f r o m w h i c h t h e y w i l l n o t r e f r a i n , u n l e s s t h e y a r e s h o w n t h e d a m a g e t h e y h a v e d o n e t o t h e i r v o c a l m e c h a n i s m . c o l o u r p h o t o g r a p h s of l a r y n g e a l n o r m a l i t y a n d a b n o r m a l i t i e s a r e s h o w n t o m a k e t h e s u b j e c t u n d e r s t a n d h i s / h e r o w n c o n d i t i o n . p e o p l e w h o h a b i t u a l l y p h o n a t e w i t h h a r d g l o t t a l o n s e t s a n d c a n n o t r e f o r m t h r o u g h a u d i t o r y o r k i n e s t h e t i c c o n t r o l w i l l , a s a r u l e , b e n e f i t f r o m t h e v i s u a l a p p r o a c h w i t h t h e l a r y n g o r e f l e c t o s c o p e . p r o f e s s i o n a l s p e a k e r s a n d s i n g e r s , v o c a l l y h a n d i c a p p e d b e c a u s e of u n d u e w o r r y a b o u t t h e i r v o i c e s , w h o a r e e x a m i n e d w i t h t h e i n s t r u m e n t , s p o n t a n e o u s l y b e c o m e r e a s s u r e d a b o u t t h e i r l a r y n x . i n h i s s p r a c h h e i l k u n d e ( p . 1 6 1 , s e q . ) h . g u t z m a n n s t a t e s t h a t c a s e s of a p h o n i a s p a s t i c a , b o t h of t h e a d d u c t o r a n d t h e a b d u c t o r t y p e , a s w e l l a s s e v e r e d y s p h e m i c s c a n f r e q u e n t l y i m p r o v e r e m a r k a b l y , if t h e y a r e t a u g h t t h e u s e of a u t o l a r y n g o s c o p i c e q u i p m e n t . 4. conclusion p r a c t i c a l e x p e r i e n c e s h o w s t h a t , t h a n k s t o t h e t r a n s p a r e n t m i r r o r a n d t h e e x t r e m e flexib i l i t y of t h e i n s t r u m e n t , t h e l a r y n g o r e f l e c t o s c o p e is a p a r t i c u l a r l y e a s y t o h a n d l e , effici e n t d e v i c e for v o i c e e x a m i n a t i o n , s t u d e n t t r a i n i n g a n d r e h a b i l i t a t i o n w o r k . summary t h e l a r y n g o r e f l e c t o s c o p e is a n i n s t r u m e n t d e s i g n e d for i n s t r u c t i n g s t u d e n t s in t h e t e c h n i q u e s of v o i c e e x a m i n a t i o n a n d is u s e d for t h e d e m o n s t r a t i o n of t h e l i v e a n a t o m y a n d p h y s i o l o g y of t h e l a r y n x . i t is a l s o u s e d t o s h o w t h e l a r y n x t o c e r t a i n v o i c e c a s e s for p e d a g o g i c a n d t h e r a p e u t i c r e a s o n s . t h e i n s t r u m e n t c a n b e u s e d w i t h a c o n t i n u o u s o r s t r o b o s c o p i c l a m p . note the laryngo-reflectoscope was designed by the author and first built in june, 1962. it was shown and/ demonstrated at the scientific exhibits of the congress of the international assocaition of logopedics and phoniatrics, at padua, aug./sept., 1962, and of the convention of the american speech and hearing association, in new york, nov.1962. journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society trends in the field of habilitation of the cerebral palsied by f. m. tragott vorwerg. a short time ago, i completed a world tour of centres for the cerebral palsied. the advantages of present day air travel are such, that i covered over 100,000 miles in 20 countries. not only did i enjoy a "world's eye" view of facilities for the cerebral palsied, but i was able to examine these facilities, exchange views with hundreds of people dedicated to the many fields of work in cerebral palsy habilitation and discuss the work we ourselves are doing in south africa. it is impossible in an article such as this to describe all i saw, or discuss at length the wealth of knowledge gained by such a unique experience. however, a few general impressions may be of interest. of outstanding importance is that those working for the cerebral palsied belong not only to their own local community, but to an international family,. the members of which want to help each other. all over the world, there are those who want to share our problems, add to our knowledge, and are keenly interested in what we are doing in the union. we have long known of advances in our work in such countries as the united states and in australia. one was elated and humbled to hear of work being done in many backward areas (e.g. asia) for the cerebral palsied. elated at the high standard of knowledge of professional workers and humbled, because limited facilities in medicine, health, social welfare and education in poorly developed countries have presented formidable obstacles which have not prevented progress. the concept of cerebral palsy habilitation has changed. it is now universally acepted that treatment and education are only the first stages of a complete programme for the cerebral palsied. education which does not lead to economic independence or a useful daily life is wasteful and in many respects a failure. in all countries, realistic thinking and planning are replacing the over-optimistic generalisations which marked the initial development of work in our field. the days of fancy slogans telling us that all the cerebral palsied can become "normal citizens", if given treatment, are passing. this misguided publicity led parents to expect the impossible and filled them with hopes which were beyond realisation. there is much bitterness and heart-break as a result. professional workers with enthusiasm and great emotional impetus to be leaders in a new field of work, find, instead of expected miracles, acute frustrations and innumerable problems which still remain unanswered. it is quite wrong to call the cerebral palsied the "world's forgotten children." no other form of handicap has received so much attention of recent years or created so many controversies. many of the cerebral palsied have received years of skilled training in all aspects of habilitation. despite this, they remain severely handicapped in locomotion, hand function, speech and scholastic ability. pioneering efforts, initiated in every instance by parents, were almost entirely devoted to children with cerebral palsy. this is understandable. these children, however, are now almost grown up. the question arose—everywhere i went: "what happens to the children leaving cerebral palsy centres who need vocational training, or care which will provide a useful daily life?" in particular, parents are demanding some assurance that their children will have understanding care if they require it, when they (the parents) are no longer living. we have this problem in south africa, and it is occupying the full attention of the cerebral palsy division of the national council for the care of cripples, who hope the establishment of a village settlement will be the answer. vocational training and employment placement of the cerebral palsied was a main topic of discussion everywhere. australia, new zealand, the united states, canada and great britain have initiated special programmes to deal with this. from all reports received it is evident that the number of cerebral palsied who can be placed in the open labour market is pitifully small. a few remarks regarding the therapies. it is almost universally accepted that physio-occupational and speech therapies are essential services in the cerebral palsy unit. however, i visited centres where occupational therapists were not employed from choice. such therapy was shared by the physio-therapist and the teacher and, i may add, very successfully. then, too, it was very noticeable in vocational workshops in the united states, that the occupational therapist was often conspicuous by her absence. training was being r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 journal of the south african logopedic society september initiated and carried out by personnel direct from the fields of industry, commerce and the like. frequently, these people had had little or no previous association with the handicapped and yet were achieving startling results. a point put to me was that for years, the cases concerned had been managed by therapists with continual attention to their handicaps. in these workshops, the handicaps are taken for granted. in an environment geared to the function of a normal factory or office, the client is concerned with his worth as a potential employee, his output as a worker, his training to do a job, despite any handicaps. more important, with new feelings of personal worth and the realization that economic independence might be within reach, improvement in skills developed almost spontaneously, and these without the specific technical attention of doctors and therapists. in the field of physiotherapy, no one system has been evolved for the treatment of cerebral palsy. the most progressive centres are guided by the needs of the individual patient. thus, he may respond to one single method or parts of many methods, or even to just the personality and common sense of a therapist with only her general training and perspicacity to guide her. il is heartening to find that those whom we accept as authorities in this field are not quite so hide-bound as of yore. each is beginning to accept the ideas of others. many physiotherapists are asking if there is not such a thing as "too much physiotherapy" for the cerebral palsied. some centres are experimenting on reducing the time given to treatment. it might be significant that in one centre, where great stress was laid on physiotherapy of a specific type (to the extent that i felt it was a discipline) there was a large number of behaviour problems among the cases. the fact that great emphasis was laid on the use of braces and appliances with consequent restriction of movement could also be a causative factor. however, i visited only one cerebral palsy centre for children where physiotherapy was not available. parents took their children to the local hospital after school hours for treatment. medical officers reported no adverse effects from this arrangement, while the education of the children benefited through less interruption in the schooling programme. the essential place of physiotherapy in the habilitation programme is beyond dispute. physical improvement in the severely handicapped cerebral palsied case under treatment is slow and rarely spectacular. those parents who doubt the value of physiotherapy services should see as i did the chronic deformities developed over the years in adult cerebral palsy cases who had not had the benefit of treatment and training. born at a time when no services were available, these men and women, fine and intelligent people, are today so deformed and handicapped as to be inmates in a home for custodial care. lack of adequate communication in speech and writing is universally recognized as one of life's greatest handicaps. the speech therapist is of vital importance in the cerebral palsy habilitation scheme. it is significant that all countries report a shortage of trained speech therapists. one no longer hears of "speech defects" but of "language disorders" which covers a much wider field. the ability to sjieak and have understanding of the written and spoken word touches on all subjects in the schooling programme. consequently, the speech therapist plays an important part in the teaching of reading, writing and arithmetic—all aspects of language development. teachers and speech therapists must work closely together, more so than we have expected in the past. in new zealand where this is fully realized, students for speech therapy courses must be qualified teachers before they are accepted for training and this is also the case in parts of australia. such "teacher-therapists" are not concerned only with speech defectives as we accept the term, but are making important contributions towards solving educational problems of so-called "normal" children in the ordinary schools. the employment of therapists in schools for the cerebral palsied by official bodies (such as government education departments) shows a varying degree of control. this varies not only from country to country but frequently from province to province within those countries. in the schools (these are better designated as habilitation centres) the controlling body is the education department. such departments accept full responsibility for educational staff (teachers). many will not accept the therapists on the same basis. it was not unusual to find that teachers are paid by an official department with accepted privileges, such as pension schemes, vacation pay, etc., while therapists have to be paid by a voluntary organization who impose their own conditions of service. sometimes two official departments are involved: eg. education and health—in which conditions of service have little in common. it is unsatisfactory as it leads to feelings of resentment and is not conducive to good teamwork. this leads to another point—what constitutes "teamwork" in the cerebral palsy unit? i visited many centres and was met with the remark— "teamwork is the basis of our success." i found on investigation that a wholistic concept of teamwork did not exist. certainly there was a good r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society team of doctors, another of therapists and another of educationalists. unfortunately the teams never met together. in one instance, the "medical" teams (employed by voluntary agencies) and the "educational teams" (employed by a state department) only communicated with each other in writing. many teachers complained to me that they were regarded as only a "necessary evil" in the habilitation programme and there was lively discussion on this point at the pan pacific conference in sydney. in the educational sphere, the most noteworthy progress is shown in the development of special techniques for teaching the brain-injured. here the united states leads the way, but valuable research is being done in great britain and france. may i add with pride that the work which has been carried out at the forest town teaching school for spastics in johannesburg for several years is equally meritorious. the controversial question of criteria of admission to cerebral palsy schools is not quite the contentious issue it used to be. where state departments control or subsidise schools for the cerebral palsied, the i.q. level of admittance is high, in most places no less than 80. children who do not fall within this "educable" category are catered for by the voluntary cerebral palsy associations. the best of these organizations do not run "centres of ineducable children" but "assessment centres." here children benefit by long or short term treatment and training and dependent on the result, placement may be made. this solves the great problem of the "border-line" case, gives the benefit of any doubt to the child, and makes possible, after careful training and observation, a more accurate placement. in south africa, most attention has been directed to "educable children." throughout the world, voluntary cerebral palsy associations have not left out the "other children." many of the socalled "ineducables" prove otherwise if given time and opportunity. it is not out of place to remind such organizations that if they are working for the cerebral palsied, the funds they raise should be utilised for the benefit of all the cerebral palsied who may need assistance and not for a selected group with this condition. many of the schools in australia have separate units for the non-educable children. the staff are paid and facilities provided by the cerebral palsy associations. with typical australian terseness, they are called "minding groups" and provide a much needed service. it should not be forgotten that the parents of the so-called ineducable cerebral palsied child need help to lighten an almost unsupportable burden. a brief mention of one or two outstanding impressions in closing:—the importance of careful diagnosis and medical assessment. south africa has much to learn here. treatment and education of cerebral palsy cases must begin at the earliest possible age. the department of education, arts, and sciences are to be congratulated on the work being done here for the pre-school group. great britain could learn from this. there are enormous sums of money devoted to cerebral palsy work in progressive countries overseas. because of this, there are better facilities for the training of staff, and equipment is plentiful and of a much higher standard than that obtainable here. while it is accepted that specially designed buildings make for better working conditions, they are not an absolute essential. i saw units which ran the gamut in accommodation— from disused garages, dark basements, old houses to the most modern buildings of special technical design. let me forever dispel the idea that only a single-storey building can be used. in great britain, such a building is hard to find and in france, one centre operates on several floors of a block of business offices. in all these places, a high standard of achievement is evident. there is no one country which can offer perfection in every aspect of cerebral palsy work and the state-aided schools in the union can hold their own with any i saw overseas. it was an honour for me to be able to describe what we are doing in south africa, as part of a great international movement. regardless of colour, race, creed, politics or geographical boundaries, men and women throughout the world are united in a common cause: to improve the welfare of the cerebral palsied; to find the cause of the condition and its prevention. to all those interested in this work, i bring good wishes and warm greetings in many languages from many friends valuable to our cause. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 73 caregiver-child interaction in a rural village in south africa belinda κ seeff and melissa a bortz department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand abstract the aim of this study was to describe the caregiver-child interaction in a south african rural village. a social interactional approach was adopted and a triangulation of methods was used. the interaction of nine caregiver-child dyads was observed during a semi-structured play situation, and the caregivers were interviewed concerning their beliefs about, and attitudes towards their child's communication. interactions were rated according to an interactional profile and common themes found during the interviews, were identified. interview results showed that although the majority of caregivers assigned importance to interacting and playing with children, their reported actions were not congruent with this. caregivers played a dominant role during the social play interactions. support was found for the utilisation of the social interactional approach and a triangulation of methods. results obtained have important implications for the south african speech-language pathologist. opsomming die doel van hierdie navorsing was om oppasser-kind-interaksies binne 'n landelike gemeenskap van suid-afrika te beskryf. nege oppasser-kind-interaksies is waargeneem tydens 'n semi-gestruktureerde spelsituasie, en onderhoude is met die oppassers gevoer, om hulle houdings en oortuigings met betrekking tot hulle kind se kommunikasie vas te stel. die interaksies is volgens 'n interaskieprofiel beskryf, en temas wat deurlopend tydens die onderhoude voorgekom het, is ge'identifiseer. uit die onderhoude het dit geblyk dat die oppassers se handelinge nie ooreeengestem het met die meederheidsmening dat spel en interaksie met kinders belangrik is nie. die oppassers het 'η dominante rol gespeel tydens die interaksies . die', resultate het die benutting van die sosiale interaksionele benadering en 'n triangulasienavorsingsmetodiek gesteun. die verkree resultate hou belangrike implikasies in vir die praktiserende spraaktaalterapeut in die suid-afrikaanse konteks. 1. introduction "the profession of speech, language and hearing therapy is under pressurej in south africa to provide services to the majority of the population" (mckenzie, 1992). however, a prerequisite to providing appropriate services, is an understanding of the cultural beliefs and practices of this majority in relation to speech and language issues. this is based on taylor's (1986, p. 16) assertion that "social and cultural factors undergird all clinical activities in speech, language and audiology". at present the lack of information regarding these issues, is seen as a major obstacle to the provision of appropriate services to the diverse, multicultural south african population. therefore, it is essential that exploratory research in this area is undertaken. upon recognising the relevance of social and cultural factors in the field of speech-language pathology and audiology, a pragmatic or social-interactive approach may be adopted when focusing on the area of language acquisition. this approach gained popularity in the late 1970's, when researchers, such as bates (1976) and snow (1977), challenged the conclusiveness of chomsky's nativist theory of language acquisition. they concluded that it is not only the nature of the child, but the nature of the social interactive setting in which language is learned, which influences the pattern of language acquisition. after long ignoring the importance of the social setting for learning, attention was turned to assessing the support for language learning that children gain from their environment, as well as from those with whom they interact. in accordance with this, numerous researchers (bruner, 1975; bullowa, 1979; kretschmer & kretschmer, 1979; snow & ferguson, 1977) have supported the notion that "competence in communicating must be learned within the course of everyday caregiving and play transactions between the child and the caregiver" (cole & st.claire-stokes, 1984, p.200). other authors have highlighted the importance of social play interaction in the acquisition of essential language and communication skills (bruner, 1986; craig & gallagher, 1986; reifel, 1992). as a result, most recent studies concerned with children's communicative interactions, condie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 belinda κ seeff & melissa a brotz centrate on describing interactions during social play (guttman & frederiksen, 1985). in addition to this, duchan (1989, p. 16) postulates that "if children learn most of their language in their everyday interactions with those in their environment, then it becomes expedient for speech-language pathologists to examine what occurs in these interactions." emphasis is placed on the dyadic nature of the adultchild interaction (garrard, 1988) and it's potential importance in the language development of the child. therefore, the study of the nature of everyday caregiving and play interactions between caregivers and children is of vital importance in the understanding of early communication development. at present, nearly all developmental and intervention data available is based on the assessment of "normal, white mainstream children from middle-class, two parent families" (westby & erickson, 1992, p . v l ) . goldberg (1977) points out that often, this western experience is taken to be normal and even when attempts are made to adapt an approach to local experience, the premises remain western. an example of this is the hanen early language parent program (manolson, 1985) which is based on traditional western assumptions concerning various cultural issues related to child language. van kleeck (1992) provides a critique of the application of this program to non-mainstream cultures. she postulates that the failure often experienced, is due to non-mainstream cultural values and beliefs, that impact on interactive patterns, opposing those of the program. therefore, this study endeavoured to establish a preliminary data base of specific non-western cultural values and beliefs. a further rationale for this study related to the dire shortage of speech-language pathologists and audiologists in south africa. aron (1991) estimated that of all the people who require speech therapy in south africa, only 1.81% receive it from the approximately 558 working therapists. this situation mirrors the world wide lack of adequate resources and personpower needed to continue providing effective speech services on a oneto-one basis. thus, the need arises to move away from the traditional, individualistic type of therapy setting and at the same time explore the feasibility of alternative intervention models. one such intervention model is the family-centred approach (westby, 1990) where the parents, and specifically the mother, become ideal candidates as intervention partners. this is based on the knowledge of many western mainstream cultures, where it is assumed that the parents, and particularly mothers, are the primary caregivers (schieffelin & eisenberg, 1984; westby, 1990). however, consideration of the south african experience, where it is common for children to spend "considerable periods of time away from one or both parents" (reynolds, 1989, p.34), highlights the need for investigation into the applicability of this type of intervention model within the south african context. in 1978, penn stressed the need to "embark on the journey of acquiring knowledge of developmental and social linguistic norms within our country, as a prerequisite for accurate diagnosis and treatment" (p.242). sixteen years later, it is clearly evident that despite the growing recognition of the impact of sociolinguistic factors on both the acquisition of, and attitudes towards communicative competence, traditional western based assessment and therapeutic procedures continue to be implemented. as the ramifications of this concept are particularly hard felt in our multi-cultural south african population, it is essential that speech-language pathologists and audiologists begin establishing a much needed database of diverse socio-cultural beliefs and practices. in light of this, the purpose of this study was to describe the nature of caregiver-child interaction, in a rural village in the eastern transvaal. 2. methodology 2.1. aims the aims of this study were: • to describe the caregivers' practices, attitudes and beliefs concerning their interaction with their children, from information gathered during interviews held with the caregivers. • to observe, transcribe and analyse the interactions between the caregivers and their children through the medium of social play. 2.2. research design an exploratory descriptive design was utilised. this design implies that a qualitative approach was adopted. in describing the holistic, qualitative approach, patton & westby (1992) assert that a description and understanding of the social environment or context is essential for an overall understanding of what is to be observed. in accordance with this, the researcher lived with the people in the village for the period of six days in which the semi-structured observations and interviews took place. the benefits of living with the people during the performance of research, proved to be two-fold: familiarity with the subjects was established. by working with the people in the context of their lives, a relationship of trust between the researcher and the subjects was encouraged (reynolds, 1989).ι this was necessary in order to secure the co-operati'on of the subjects, which giddens (1989) views as being an essential factor in increasing the reliability of the data to be obtained. | • in addition to being able to observe the subjects in their natural environment prior to the implementation of the research, the researcher was able to sketch a general description of the village and its inhabitants. 2.3. description of village as a result of the absence of any official information pertaining to the village, a brief description of the village and its inhabitants, as seen through the eyes of the researcher, is provided. , / manzini village is one of the villages situated in the nsikazi region of the eastern transvaal, located between the towns of white river and nelspruit. the predominant language spoken in the village is siswati. the village consists of mud, brick and corrugated iron houses, on small plots founcl scattered on the hills characteristic of the area. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41,1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) caregiver-child interaction in a rural village in south africa 75 as no farm land is available to the people, they are unable to generate their own resources. through discussion with the village dwellers, it was established that the village is seen as a labour reserve for the surrounding farms, as well as the nearest towns. of the people who remain in the village, income is generated by activities such as selling petty goods, running shebeens and repairing cars. however, it was observed that the majority of people living in the village were unemployed. as many of the men work varying distances away from the village, the daily population of the village is comprised largely of women, children and those adolescents who have finished or left school, and cannot find work. the daily activities of the women mainly consisted of cleaning the inside and outside of their houses, fetching water, cooking food and sitting under a tree talking to their neighbours. it was observed that the large population of children generally played in groups of varying ages, as they wandered from house to house. in accordance with reynold's (1989) findings during research on a squatter settlement, adults were rarely observed to be involved in the activities of the children. also in common with reynold's (1989) findings, were the materials used by the children during play. the main materials used included balls and cars; a variety of items found in the environment, such as stones, sticks, water, tins and bottles; as well as items that were found in their homes, such as percussion drums and spades. 2.4. subjects a purposive quota sampling procedure was used when selecting subjects (cohen & manion, 1991). information presented in table 1, shows that the age of the children range from 2.11 years to 3.9 years, with a mean age of 3.4 years. this age range was selected as the fundamentals of communicative behaviour are mostly present by this stage (gesell, 1978). the intellectual functioning, hearing and visual acuity of each child was informally assessed by the researcher, as well as the caregiver involved, to be within normal limits. although it has been shown that the education of the caregiver may have an effect on "several aspects of child's health" (yach, ricliter, cameron & dewet 1993, table 1. description ofjsubjects dyad age of caregiver age of child 1 child's gender language education level of caregiver 1 24 yrs 3.4 yrs f siswati std. 3 2 29 yrs 2.11 yrs μ siswati std. 7 3 29 yrs 3.0 yrs f siswati std. 9 4 37 yrs 3.0 yrs μ siswati std. 3 5 30 yrs 3.7 yrs μ siswati std. 6 6 32 yrs 3.8 yrs f siswati std. 8 7 22 yrs 3.0 yrs f siswati std. 3 8 32 yrs 3.6 yrs μ siswati std. 3 9 30 yrs 3.9 yrs μ siswati std. 2 die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 p.22), prior observation revealed the difficulty in imposing the criteria of age and education. therefore, although no specific age range and education level were specified, the researcher attempted to limit the range of these variables as much as possible. the age of the caregivers ranged from 22-39 years, with a mean age of 30 years. the caregivers had no previous training in speech and language or related areas. 2.5. methods and procedures in order to determine the nature of mother-child interaction during play, a triangulation of methods was employed (mcneill, 1990). this implies that more than one research method was used in "an attempt to strengthen the validity of empirical evidence by reliance on more than one approach" (bulmer, 1991, p.45). following a pilot study, using one caregiver-child dyad, the following two research methods and procedures were adopted: 2.5.1. interviews when addressing issues relating to children of diverse cultures, westby (1990) postulates that interviewing provides a means for understanding the environment of the child, as well as the perceptions and values of the families concerned. semi-structured interviews were utilised in order to describe the practices, attitudes and beliefs of the caregivers in relation to their interaction with their children. this implies that although questions were preset, the researcher was free to modify the sequencing and wording of the questions (cohen & manion, 1991). this facilitated greater understanding on the part of the caregiver, and hence increased research reliability. in accordance with the naturalistic component of qualitative research, no attempt was made to manipulate, control or eliminate situational variables (westby, 1992). thus, interviews took place both inside and outside the houses, depending on the preference of the caregiver involved. both open and closed ended questions were used. as the researcher's knowledge of siswati was limited, a siswati speaking researcher was used as an inr ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 belinda κ seeff & melissa a brotz terpreter during the interviews. the questions were asked by the interpreter, and the responses given by the c a r e g i v e r s were i m m e d i a t e l y transcribed orthographically. in order to fulfil the first aim of this study, specific questions were asked in an attempt to gain an understanding of the caregivers' beliefs, attitudes and practices in relation to the following issues: • the content of interaction with the child. • the value assigned to talking to children. • how children acquire language abilities. • the value assigned to children talking. • the value assigned to playing with children. • the quantity of play interaction between the caregivers and their children. • the types of games and materials used during play interaction between the caregivers and their children. mcneill (1990) points out that there is no guarantee that what people say in interviews is a true account of what they actually do. this potential weakness inherent in the interviewing method, was addressed by the utilisation of the triangulation of methods. thus semistructured observations of the caregiver-child interactions during social play, were performed. 2.5.2. observations semi-structured observations were employed in order to fulfil the second aim of the study of transcribing and analysing the interaction between the caregivers and their children during social play. in the context of this study, the term 'semi structured' implies that although the situation was structured in that the caregiver was requested to play with the child, the course of the caregiver-child interaction was not controlled or predetermined by the researcher. in accounting for the possible weakness of subjectivity often associated with observation methods (bless & achola, 1990), video recordings were utilised. this served to increase the objectivity and hence the reliability and validity of the study. according to cole & st.claire-stokes (1984) a video analysis procedure systematically examines specific behaviours of both the caregiver and the child as they interact. a phillips video recorder (model vkr 684/00), using 6v panasonic batteries, powered by a portable generator, was used. based on the prior observation of the children at play, as well as the pilot study, an attempt was made to provide each dyad with situationally appropriate play materials. these materials included metal tins, sticks, sand, water, a toy car, a ball and bottles. however, it was noted that during the play interactions, the caregivers and their children often used other objects within their environment, such as small planks of wood, a scarf, string, cardboard boxes, and plastic ice-cream containers. this was viewed as a strength of the exploratory nature of the research design in that strictly specifying and limiting play materials to be used, would have been indicative of the researcher's bias, and hence negatively effect the reliability of the study. the video recordings took place in a secluded area, outside the subjects' houses. this was done in order to ensure adequate lighting needed to achieve quality video recordings, as well as to prevent interruptions caused by remarks and questions by the curious onlookers. the total interaction time of each caregiver-child dyad was approximately 20 minutes. in recognising the cultural inappropriateness associated with using a video recorder, recording equipment was set up, but actual recording only began after approximately five minutes. this time was necessary for the subjects to become familiar with the recording set-up, as well as the procedure. the recording procedure required the researcher to be present during the caregiver-child interactions. although familiar with the subjects, the researcher was aware of williamson, karp, dalphin & gray's (1982, p.207) assertion that it is impossible to "observe human beings without influencing their behaviour". the researcher was aware that as she was from a different culture, this may have further influenced the subjects' behaviour. in order to maximise the representativeness of the video recorded sample, information and instructions, adapted from cole & st.claire-stokes (1984), were given to each caregiver (see appendix a). 2.6. processing and analysis of data the processing and analysis of the data obtained was dependent on the research method utilised. 2.6.1. interviews answers to questions were translated by the interpreter into english during the interview. the data was then transcribed orthographically. in accordance with cohen and manion's (1991) procedure of analysing interview data, the researcher identified common themes, as well as individual variations, in the beliefs and practices of the caregivers regarding interaction with their children. 2.6.2. semi-structured observations data obtained from the semi-structured observations was transcribed orthographically by two trained people whose first language was siswati. the data was ithen translated into english by the interpreter. j in keeping with the social-interactional model adopted, focus was placed on analysing certain interactional components of the caregiver-child interactions. in line with the exploratory nature of this study, the profile of interactional behaviours was only drawn up after the researcher had lived with and observed the people, and obtained the video recorded data from the pilot study. thus, the researcher was not confined to preconceived variables and the inclusion of new interactional variables was made possible (salter ainsworth, 1977). • interactional profile the interactional behaviours and their descriptions are presented in appendix b. this profile was adapted from bedrosian, wanska, sykes,,smith & dalton (1988) and conti-ramsden & dykins (1991). the interactional behaviours were classified into verbal and non-verbal behaviours. in addition, a breakthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41,1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) caregiver-child interaction in a rural village in south africa 77 down of the total number of utterances made by the caregivers and children was also calculated. it must be noted that it was possible for an 'utterance' to be analysed a number of times into different categories. frequency counts were done and percentages calculated by the researcher. • inter-rater reliability . thirty percent of the data was randomly selected and independently recoded as suggested by contiramsden & dykins (1991). two raters, a qualified speech-language pathologist and a final year speechlanguage pathology student, performed this task. this suggestion, made by conti-ramsden & dykins (1991) was followed in order to reduce experimental bias and i n c r e a s e the reliability of the study (silverman, 1977). raters were familiarised with the required analysis scheme by means of oral instructions. they were required to make coded judgements relating to the verbal and non-verbal behaviours of the caregivers and children. inter-rater reliability of between 75-100% was obtained utilising the following equation provided by mcreynolds & kearns (1983, p.86): number of agreement 100 total number of agreements and disagreements 1 = % agreement 3. results and discussion the results obtained from interviews and observations, and the discussion thereof, are presented below. 3.1. interviews • the content of caregiver-child interaction the results illustrating the content of the caregiverchild interaction, as perceived by the caregivers, are presented in figure 1 below.] it, should be noted that the nature of the question allowed caregivers to respond positively to more than one category. the content of interactions, as reported by the caregivers, consists mainly of instructions given by the caregiver, with very few questions or stories featuring in these interactions. the four'caregivers, who reported that they mainly answer questions when interacting with their child, all commented that they do not interact with their child unless the child asks questions. τ η g i v e i _> j , l _ s. ι ask tetl stories a n s w e r instructions questions questions • the value assigned to talking to children five out of the nine caregivers thought it important to speak to their children. three caregivers thought it unimportant to speak to their children, and one caregiver was unsure of what to answer. bearing in mind the fact that results in figure 1 show interactions to consist mainly of instructions, it is interesting to note that five caregivers thought it important to speak to their children besides when instructing them or sending them on errands. these caregivers suggested the following reasons for speaking to children: "the caregiver shows her love for the child." "the relationship between the caregiver and the child would be improved so that the child will no longer be scared of the caregiver." "the child learns new things and it helps him or her to grow. you can show them the way in life." "the caregiver can tell if the child is ill and if he or she can hear properly." it can therefore be seen that the value that caregivers assigned to talking to their children related to issues of wellbeing, relationships and education. • how children acquire language abilities all nine caregivers believed that a child learns to talk through interacting with other people. however, a number of caregivers highlighted various aspects of interaction, as being crucial to language acquisition. one caregiver identified the interaction during play with other children as a contributing factor to the child's speech development. another caregiver highlighted the role of the child him/herself in language acquisition. she proposed that by interacting with other people and asking questions, the child learns how to talk. furthermore, two caregivers highlighted the mother or caregiver as playing a major role in the language acquisition of the child. they explained that the child learns to talk when he or she is in the mother's lap. the mother's role is to talk to the child during this period, before it is time for the child to go and play with other children. the caregivers stated that this occurs at approximately two years of age, when the child begins to speak. one caregiver stated that it is at this age that the mother begins to shape the child's speech. analysis of the responses given by the caregivers show that there are similarities between their attitudes— and beliefs relating to language acquisition, and traditional western beliefs. among these similarities is the support for the social-interactional approach to language acquisition (bates, 1976). the influence of the caregiver in particular, on the child's language acquisition, is also in accordance with western researchers who have proposed that there are special properties of a mother's speech that play a significant role in language acquisition (bernstein & tiegerman, 1989). in addition to this, the notion expressed by the caregivers, that at two years of age the child begins to talk, is supported by the western language universal concept that children begin to understand and say words in approximately their second year of life (bloom & lahey, 1978). figure 1. content of caregiver-child interaction. die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 belinda κ seeff & melissa a brotz • the value assigned to children talking figure 2 illustrates that only one out of the nine caregivers interviewed, thought it unimportant for a child to learn to talk. she claimed that "the child will end up talking unnecessary things." the remaining eight caregivers reacted favourably to the idea of their children talking a lot. they felt that it is through talking a lot and asking questions that a child learns about the world around him/her. two caregivers believed that it shows intelligence when a child talks a lot, while one caregiver thought it important as "then the child will be able to cope at school and become clever." once again, similarities between the caregivers' values and beliefs and traditional western values are highlighted: van kleeck (1992) asserts that talking is a behaviour highly valued in most western cultures. this is also in accordance with demuth's observations (1986) of the basotho community where caregivers assigned great value to talking. the view expressed that talking is seen as a vehicle for further growth and learning, is related to schiefelbusch & bricker's (1981) assertion that the acquisition of language allows for the development of knowledge and functions that are essential for future learning. • the value assigned to playing with children figure 3 shows that eight out of nine caregivers assigned importance to playing with their children. practical reasons, as well as those relating to relationships not important 1 ά ιϊΐϊίτΐί 1 ^ u u i i important 8 figure 2. the value of children talking. and cognitive development were given by the caregivers to support their beliefs. these included: "playing with your child shows your love for it." "it keeps the child at home so that they won't wander off into other peoples' homes." three caregivers viewed play as an activity which " r e s u l t s in advances in c o g n i t i v e d e v e l o p m e n t " (guttman & frederiksen, 1985, p. 165). one caregiver proposed that playing will help the child in his school work. she explained that "if a child learns how to build a house, then at school he will know how to draw it." another caregiver stated that "it helps the child develop intelligence." three caregivers saw play as a means to ensure the successful performance of household chores in the future. this idea was elaborated upon by one caregiver who stated that "the child learns something for the future in the home situation she learns to cook and do household chores." in support of this, reynolds (1989) points out that children in non-western societies learn by 'observation and imitation', and thus imitate adults in their play in preparation for adult roles. • the quantity of caregiver-child play interaction five caregivers reported to play with their children. the amount of time spent playing ranged from less than 20 minutes per day, to less than 20 minutes per month. only one caregiver reported to play with her child for more than 20 minutes per day. four caregivers reported that they did not play with their children. the reasons given for not playing with their children are presented in table 2. it is significant to note that although eight caregivers assigned importance to playing with their children, their reported practices did not appear to be congruent with their attitudes and beliefs. the researcher hypothesises that the short and infrequent play interactions that reportedly take place, may be explained by the attitude expressed by the fourth caregiver. she believed that she would loose her respected status if she played with her child. this is directly related to reynold's (1989) observations that she made whilst observing childrenjin a squatter settlement near cape town. she observed that few adults involved themselves in the play of children, as this was regarded as being undignified. | table 2. reasons for not playing with children n o t important 1 important 8 figure 3. the value of playing with children. the south caregiver reason for not playing with children vi "i have too much work." vii "i don't know what to do when playing with the child." viii "i have time to play but never think that i should." iv "there is time to play, but the mother will loose her respected status if she plays with the child people will think that you are not right in your head,." ican journal of communication disorders, vol. 41,1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) caregiver-child interaction in a rural village in south africa 79 • types of games played and materials used the caregivers stated that the games that they play with their children consist primarily of ball games, sangoma games, and a variety of domestic activities such a s cooking, washing and building. it is significant to note that the domestic games played are in accordance with the view expressed by the caregivers that play is a medium through which children learn to perform household chores. the materials that the caregivers reported to use during play, corresponded to those used by the children in reynold's (1989) study. these materials could be classified into ready made toys, such as a skipping rope, a ball and a car; and those found in the environment and adapted for play, such as water, sand and tins. 3.2. semi-structured observation 3.2.1. verbal behaviours • total number of utterances results displayed in table 3 show that the caregivers were more verbal than the children. they were responsible for at least two thirds of the total utterances spoken, with an average percentage of 80%. this can be compared to the average of 20% of the total utterances made by the children. one caregiver-child interaction served to epitomise the disproportionate number of caregiver utterances, as compared to the number of utterances said by the child. here, 99% of the total number of utterances were made by the caregiver, whereas the child only said one utterance throughout the interaction. • spontaneous vs response utterances table 4 shows that 80% of the caregivers' utterances were spontaneous. this implies that the caregivers seldomly responded to a previous action, question, instruction or statement made by the children. when the table 3. total number of subjects' utterances table 4. spontaneous versus response utterances caregivers did respond to the children, almost all of the responses were verbal (99%). during one caregiver-child interaction (no. 8), the caregiver responded to the child only four times out of her total of 74 utterances. during this interaction it appeared as if a combination of a lack of spontaneity and interest on the part of the child, as well as the preoccupation of the caregiver in her own activities, contributed to a general lack of responsiveness from both parties. a general lack of spontaneity on the part of the children was noted, as only an average 19% of their total utterances were spontaneous. thus, the majority of the utterances made by the children (81%) were in response to an action, instruction, question or statement made by the caregivers. it is significant to note that out of the total 21 response utterances made by the children, at least half of the utterances were in reply to questions asked by the caregivers. • topic maintenance and shift a general interpretation of the mean percentages of topic maintenance and topic shift on the part of the caregivers, shows an expected proportion of topic shifts as compared to topic maintenance. however, closer inspection of the range percentages associated with individual caregivers, reveals that a disproportionate amount of topic shifts occurred during one interaction. here the caregiver used 58% of her utterances to shift topics and only 42% to maintain them. on the whole, the caregivers shifted topics almost three times more than the children. this is in contradiction to conti-ramsden & friel-patti's (1991) findings that normal, western, middle-class children shifted the topics approximately three times more than their mothers did in a naturalistic play setting. in addition to this, conti-ramsden & friel-patti (1991) assert that by introducing fewer topics than their children, mothers adjust their conversation style to match their child's, and allow the child to influence the flow of information exchanged in the conversation. as this did not occur in the course of any of the interactions observed, a lack of adjustment on the part of the caregivers, was demonstrated. • function of utterances: • instructions: table 6 indicates that on average, nearly half of the caregivers' utterances functioned as instructions. these results support the impression gained from the intertable 5. topic maintenance and topic shift caregivers children range mean range mean spontaneous utterances % 63-95% 80% 0-38% 19% raw score (50) (4) response utterances % 5-37% 20% 62-100% 81% raw score (12) (21) caregivers children range mean range mean topic maintenance % 42-83% 65% 79-100% 89% raw score (39) (15) topic shift % 17-58% 35% 0-21% 11% raw score (23) (2) caregivers children range mean range mean utterance total % 66-99% i 80% 1-34% 20% raw score (63) (16) die suid afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 belinda κ seeff & melissa a brotz views where the caregivers indicated that the majority of their interactions with their children take the form of instructions. they also relate to harkness's findings (1977) regarding the speech of mothers and children in the kokwet community in western kenya. she found that mothers or caregivers relied on instructions for communication. furthermore, van kleeck (1992) cites non-western, rural black communities in america who believe that it is the adult's role to issue directives and the child's role to obey them. the general lack of assertiveness on the part of the children was illustrated as only an average of 11% of their utterances served as instructions. • statements: on average, just over one third of the caregivers' utterances were statements, whereas almost half of the children's utterances were statements. this is in agreement with harkness's findings that "children's communication is more often in the form of statements" (harkness, 1977, p.313). • questions: results in table 6 show that the relative proportion of questions asked by the caregivers was approximately half that of the children. snow (1977) points out that questions serve the purpose of language elicitation in the process of facilitating language acquisition. therefore, the relatively small number of questions asked by the caregivers becomes significant in terms of the role that these caregivers play in the language acquisition of their children. ' in addition to this, 54% of the total number of questions asked by the caregivers, were not answered. it was noted during a number of interactions that the caregivers asked numerous questions without allowing any time for the child to respond, before producing the next utterance. this fact, together with the subjective observation by the researcher, leads to the conclusion that the majority of questions asked by the caregivers were probably used rhetorically. it is interesting to note that 44% of the total number of questions asked by the children served the purpose of asking for clarification. extreme examples of this were found in two caregiver-child interactions (no.6 & no.8) where all the questions asked by the children were requests for clarification. 3.2.2. non-verbal behaviours results in table 7 reveal almost no non-verbal responses from the caregivers. only one caregiver responded once non-verbally to an action performed by her child. in contrast to this are the number of nonverbal responses on the part of the children. the majority of the children's responses were non-verbal and related to instructions given by the caregivers. 3.3. general discussion the results obtained from the interviews and observations serve to support the social-interactional approach to language acquisition. it is evident that the interactions between caregivers and their children in the manzini village facilitate the acquisition of communicative competence that is appropriate to the specific social and cultural c o m m u n i c a t i o n context (schiefelbusch & pickar, 1984). as taylor & payne (1994) point out, cultural styles of verbal interaction do not impede language development, but may manifest in varying patterns of verbal expression. this has particular relevance in the south african context where a multitude of different cultures p r o v i d e diverse interactional environments which in turn result in varying communicative behaviours. thus, the need to investigate these interactional environments to determine the nature of communication expected, is highlighted. the fact that results obtained from the performance of the two research methods employed were congruent with one another, serves to highlight the strength inherent in triangulation. thus, the reliability of the study was increased and the researcher was able to gain greater insight into the phenomena under study (bless & achola, 1990). this was illustrated when the caregivers' belief that through play "the child learns household chores for the future", was supported by the observation that most of the activities that took place during the caregiver-child interactions consisted of performing household chores. in line with this^ the researcher is of the opinion that by observing and imitati ι table 7. non-verbal responses j table 6. function of subjects' utterances caregivers children range mean range mean instructions % 27-73% 44% 0-36% 11% raw score (26) (2) statements % 6-55 % 37% 0-88% . 48% raw score (23) (10) questions % 2-34% 19% 9-100% 34% raw score (12) (4) the south caregivers children 1 range mean range !mean response to question % 0% 0% 0-7% 1 2% i raw score (0) (.3) response to instruction % 0% 0% 8-83% 37% raw score (0) (7) response to action % 0-13% λ% 0-6% / raw score (λυ (.1) response to statement % 0% / ' o % 0-7% 1.8% raw score (0) (.3) ican journal of communication disorders, vol. 41,1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) caregiver-child interaction in a rural village in south africa 81 ing perceived adult roles during play, the children learn what they need to know in order to become "competent members of their society" (skinner, 1989, p.181). finally, it is interesting to note that a difference occurred between the caregiver's interactions with female children as compared to male children. upon analysing the interactions, it became evident that an interaction between a caregiver and a male child tended towards a parallel play situation, whereby the caregiver focused on her own activities and encouraged the male child to perform male orientated tasks, such as building a house or garage. more interaction took place between a caregiver and a female child as they focused on a common activity such as washing or cooking. based on these findings, the researcher agrees with rogoff's (1982) assertion that through everyday activities and social interactions, culturally shared meanings pertaining to gender are constructed and internalised. 4. summary in this study, the nature of caregiver-child interaction in the rural village of manzini has been described using semi-structured observation and interviewing methods. the following phenomena were found: 4.1. interviews the majority of the caregivers thought it important for their children to talk, which they learn to do through interacting with people in their environment. although playing with children was generally viewed as being important, short and infrequent play interactions take place between some of the caregivers and their children. the content of interaction between the caregivers and their children consists mainly of instructions given by the caregivers. the type of games which the caregivers play with their children, are mainly centred around domestic activities. play materials consist primarily of objects found in the environment. 4.2. semi-structured observations the caregivers appeared to interact more with the female children than with the male children. a dominant role was played by the caregivers during the play interactions with their children. this was demonstrated in the following ways: • they mainly instructed the children and often asked questions without allowing time for the child to reply· they responded minimally to actions, questions, instructions or statements made by the children. • they shifted topics often and were responsible for the majority of utterances spoken. the children generally took on a submissive and obedient role as was illustrated in the following ways: they made few utterances. • they tended to maintain the topics introduced by the caregivers. • their main interactional behaviour consisted of nonverbal responses to instructions given by the caregivers. 5. conclusions and implications the clinical implications of this study are highlighted by the current cultural integration taking place within south africa. as a result, the western urban clinician is often faced with the task of diagnosing patients from non-western cultures. therefore, it is essential that speech-language pathologists and audiologists do not fall into the trap of adopting the attitude that "what is 'normal' in our culture is 'right', and that cultures that are 'different' are likely to be 'wrong' or inferior" (tulkin 1977, p.569). although ideas of improving the current models of assessment have been entertained over the past few years (taylor, payne, kay & anderson, 1987), this study provides concrete evidence to highlight the urgency of researching and developing culturally valid and sensitive assessment tools. the views and corresponding actions of the manzini caregivers have particular relevance when considering the possibility of utilising these caregivers as intervention partners in the therapeutic process. although it is not possible to categorically oppose the utilisation of these caregivers as intervention partners in the traditional western milieu, the researcher cautions that the values held by the caregivers living in the manzini village are not entirely congruent with those held by western urban speech-language pathologists. accordingly, it should not be assumed that if these caregivers were to be shown a different way of interacting with their children, they would respond positively to changing their present communicative practices. furthermore, the ethical implications of western urban clinicians inviting themselves into a rural village, and introducing communication behaviours that are "good and right... and in the interest of progress" (goldberg, 1977, p.592) need to be highlighted. although acknowledging the good intentions of the western urban clinicians, goldberg (1977, p.593) points out that "the greatest harm is often done by those who think that they are doing good". by imposing the values of western cultures on this rural community, the clinicians would encourage the genocide of the community's communicative practices (crago, 1992), and hence their cultural attitudes, meanings and values that underlie these behaviours. the above notwithstanding, it is crucial that the large rural population within our country, is not ignored. rather, culturally sensitive intervention should take place in the rural context, whereby the urban clinician must be acutely aware of his/her own cultural bias which may affect the type of intervention he/she hopes to provide. communicative beliefs and practices should be exchanged between the urban clinician and the rural caregivers. by doing this, the present reciprocal ignorance, which "frequently translates into prejudice and apathy" (adler, 1979, p.203) would be overcome. in addition, the caregivers would be able to make informed decisions concerning their beliefs and practices. furthermore, whilst acknowledging the obstacles that cross-cultural communicative differences present for the speech-language pathologist, in the pursuit of appropriate assessment and intervention procedures, it die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 41, 1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 b e l i n d a κ seeff & m e l i s s a a b r o t z is crucial t h a t l a n g u a g e universale are n o t overlooked. it is the k n o w l e d g e and application of these universale, which m a y p r o v i d e a k e y to the formulation and i m p l e m e n t a t i o n o f a p p r o p r i a t e a s s e s s m e n t a n d intervention p r o g r a m m e s . a s the s u n s h i n e s b r i g h t l y on a n e w s o u t h africa ( a r c h b i s h o p d e s m o n d t u t u , 19 a p r i l 1 9 9 4 ) , w e as s p e e c h l a n g u a g e pathologists are faced with the challenge of p r o v i d i n g effective services to the majority o f the s o u t h african population. as the present study has clearly d e m o n s t r a t e d , this c h a l l e n g e can only be m e t if we c o n t i n u e o n the long b u t r e w a r d i n g j o u r n e y to acquiring k n o w l e d g e o f attitudes, beliefs and practices o f all cultural g r o u p s w i t h i n our country. r e f e r e n c e s adler,s. (1979) . poverty children and their language: implications for teaching and treating. sydney. grune and stratton. aron.m.l. (1991). perspectives. south african journal of communication disorders. vol 38. 3-11. bates,e. (1976). language and context: the acquisition of pragmatics. new york. academic press. bedrosian.j.l., wanska,s.k., sykes.k.m., smith,a.j., & dalton,b.m. (1988). conversational turn-taking violations in mother-child interactions. journal of speech and hearing research. vol 31. 81-86. bernstein,d.k. & tiegerman,e. (1989). language and communication disorders in children. (2nd ed). columbus oh. charles ε merrill. bless,c. & achola,p. (1990). fundamentals of social research methods: an african perspective. lusaka government printer. bloom,l & lahey,m. (1978). language development and language disorders. new york: john wiley and sons. bruner,j.s. (1975). the ontogenesis of speech acts. journal of child language. 2. 1-21. bullowa.m. (1979). before speech. london. cambridge university press. bulmer.m. (1991). introduction: problems, theories and methods in sociology how do they inter-relate? in b.bozzoli : study package: social/industrial sociology iii: planning and executing an research project. university of the witwatersrand. cohen,l. & manion.l. (1991)..researc/i methods in education. (3rd ed). london. routledge press. cole.e.b. & st.claire-stokes,j. (1984). caregiver-child interactive behaviours: a videotape analysis procedure. volta review. c o n t i r a m s d e n , g . & dykins,j. (1991). mother-child interactions with language-impaired children and their siblings. british journal of disorders of communication. vol 26. 337-354. conti-ramsden.g. & friel-patti,s. (1991). mother-child dialogues: a comparison of normal and language impaired children. journal of communication disorders. vol 17. 1936. crago,m.b. (1992). "ethnography and language socialisation: a cross cultural perspective. topics in language disorders. 12(3). 28-39. aspen publisher inc. craig,h.k. & gallagher,t.m. (1986). interactive play the frequency of related verbal responses. journal of speech and hearing research. vol 29. 375-383. demuth,k. (1986). prompting routines in the language socialisation of basotho children. in b.schieffelin & e.ochs (eds.) language socialisation across cultures. cambridge university press. duchan,j.f. (1989). evaluating adults talk to children: assessing adult attunement. seminars in speech and language. vol 10. 1. garrard,k.r. (1988). a mother's language to her twins: one child downs syndrome and one child with normal development. journal of childhood communication disorders. vol 12. 1. gesell,a. (1978). the first five years of life. a guide to the study of the preschool child. london. methuen and co. giddens,a. (1989). working with sociology: methods of research. in a.giddens : sociology. london. policy press. goldberg,s. (1977). ethics, politics and multicultural research. in p.h.liederman, s.r. tulkin & a.rosenfeld (eds.). culture and infancy: variations in the human experience. new york. academic press. guttman.m. & frederiksen.c.h. (1985). preschool children's narratives: linking story comprehension, production and play discourse. in l.galda & a.d. pellegrini (eds.)(1985). play, language and stories: the development of children's literature behaviour. usa. ablex publishing corporation. harkness,s. (1977). aspects of social environment and first language acquisition in rural africa. in c.snow & c.a.ferguson (eds.). talking to children: language input and acquisition. london. cambridge university press. kretschmer,r.r. & kretschmer.l.w. (1979). language development and intervention with the hearing impaired. baltimore. university park. manolson,a. (1985). it takes two to talk: a hanen early language parent guide book. (2nd revision). toronto. hanen early language resource centre. mckenzie,j.a. (1992). the provision of speech, language and hearing services in a rural district of south africa. south african journal of communication disorders. vol 39. 5061. mcneill,p. (1990) : research methods. 2nd ed. suffolk london. richard clay ltd. mcreynolds,l.r. & kearns,k.p. (1983). single subject experimental designs in communicative disorders. baltimore. university park press. patton,m. & westby,c. (1992). ethnography and research: a qualitative view. topics in language disorders. 12(3). 114. penn,c. (1978). speech pathology and audiology in south africa past, present and future p e r s p e c t i v e s . in l.lanham & k.prinsloo : current issues and directions in research and inquiry. cape town. oxford university press. reynolds,p. (1989). childhood in crossroads: cognition and society in south africa. claremont. david phillip publisher pty. ltd. reifel,s. (1992). play and language in early childhood introduction. play and culture. vol 5. 2. 103-106. rogoff,b. (1982). i n t e g r a t i n g context and c o g n i t i v e development. in m.e.lamb & a.l.brown (eds.). advances in developmental psychology. vol 2. new york. hillsdale. lea. salter ainsworth.m.d. (1977). infant development and mother-infant interaction among uganda and american families. in d.h.liederman, s.r.tulkin & a.rosenfeld : culture and infancy: variations in the human experience. new york. academic press. ! schiefelbusch.r. & pickar,j. (1984). the acquisition of communicative competence. baltimore. university i park press. i schiefelbusch.r.l. & bricker.d. (eds.)(1981). early language acquisition and intervention. baltimore. university jpark press. | schieffelin.b.b. & eisenberg.a.r. (1984). cultural variation in children's conversation. in r.schiefelbusch & j.pickar. the acquisition of communicative competence. baltiiiiore. university park press. silverman,f.h. (1977). research procedures in speech pathology and audiology. new jersey. prentice-hall. skinner,d. (1989). the socialisation of gender identity: observations from nepal. in j.valsiner (ed.). child development in cultural context. toronto. hogrefe and huber publishers. snow,c. (1977).mother's speech research: from input to interaction. in c.snow & / c.a.ferguson (eds.). talking to children: language input and acquisition. london. cambridge university press. snow,c. and ferguson,c.a. (eds.)(1977). talking to children: language input and acquisition.london. cambridge university press. ^ taylor,o.l. (ed.)(1986). nature of communication disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse populations. san ' diego. college hill press. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 41,1994 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) caregiver-child interaction in a rural village in south africa 83 taylor,o.l., payne,k,t., kay.t. & anderson,n.b. (1987). distinguishing between communication disorders and communication differences. seminars in speech and language. vol 8. 4. tavlor ο l. & payne, k.t. (1994). language and communication differences, in g.h. shames, ε. h. wiig & w.a. secord (eds.). human communication disorders: an introduction (4th ed.). p. 136-173. new york: macmillan publishing company. tulkin.s.r. (1977). dimensions of multicultural research in infancy and e a r l y c h i l d h o o d . in d . h . l i e d e r m a n , s.r.tulkin & a.rosenfeld (eds.). culture and infancy • variations in the human experience. new york. academic press inc. tutu,d. (1994) : tv1 news. jhb. sabc. van kleeck,a. (1992). future trends in language intervention: addressing cultural bias in service delivery. south african journal of communication disorders. vol 39. westby,c.e. (1990). ethnographic interviewing: asking the right questions to the right people in the right ways. journal of childhood communication disorders. vol 13(1). 101-113. westby,c. & erickson.j. (1992). prologue. topics in language disorders. 12(3). v-vii. appendix a information and instructions given to caregivers williamson, j.b., karp.d.a., dalphin, j.r., & gray.p.s. (1982) : the research craft: an introduction to social research methods. boston. little, brown and co. yach,d., richter,l.cameron,n. & dewet,t. (1993). maternal education and child health: emerging evidence from birth to ten. paper presented at birth-ten seminar on methods and findings of the first years. witwatersrand medical school. rsa. address correspondence to ms b. seeff, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, ρ ο wits, 2050, johannesburg, south africa. acknowledgments the authors would like to thank ms μ schneider, senior clinical tutor, university of the witwatersrand, for her valuable assistance in conceptualising and formulating this study. behaviourtype description verbal utterance ! / a verbal expression consisting of a word, phrase or sentence. this does not include utterances expressed while singing. spontaneous an utterance not related to the previous action, statement, instruction or question of the interaction partner. response an utterance related to the previous action, statement, instruction or question of the interaction partner. topic maintenance , continued engagement with the previous set of concerns. instruction an utterance requiring an action from the listener. — question a request for information, opinion, permission, conformation or clarification. — statement/comment an utterance which contains information about a topic or an ongoing action. non-verbal response a non-verbal reaction related to the previous action, statement, instruction or question of the interaction partner. adapted from bedrosian, wanska, sykes, smith & dalton (1988) and conti-ramsden & dykins (1991). die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 41, 1994 each caregiver was told that the researcher's aim was to observe how she and the child played together. the interpreter used a direct translation of the word 'play', that is 'dlala'. the interpretation of this was left up to the caregiver. it was further explained to each caregiver, that the researcher's intention was not to assess whether the caregiver's interaction with her chilli was right or wrong, and therefore their interaction should be as natural as possible. .each caregiver was informed that the researcher would not interact with her during the recording. appendix β r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 the communicative effectiveness index: its use with south african stroke patients claire penn and kelly milner department of speech pathology and audiology peter fridjhon department of statistics university of the witwatersrand abstract the functional communication of a group of 28 south african stroke patients was examined using the communicative effectiveness index (ceti). it was translated into afrikaans, sotho and zulu and administered to the significant others of 22 aphasic patients with left hemisphere damage and 6 patients with right hemisphere damage. results were related to the results of standardised language testing and to case history factors such as cultural factors and time since onset. the ceti was readministered in the case of eight of the aphasic subjects after a mean period of six months in order to assess its sensitivity to recovery. results showed that the ceti seems applicable across different language groups, that it is sensitive to change across time as well as sensitive to the communication disorders resulting from both right and left hemisphere damage. further it appears to correlate well with overall level of severity. it does not appear to differentiate patients in terms of time since onset. its potential use as a relatively culture free assessment tool in the south african context is discussed. opsomming diefunksionele kommunikasie van 28 suid afrikaanse pasiente is deur middel van die kommunikasie-effektiwiteitsindeks (kei) ondersoek. die indeks is in afrikaans, sotho en zulu vertaal en op die familielede van 22 afasie pasiente met linker-hemisfeerskade en 6 pasiente met regter-hemisfeerskade toegepas. die resultate is, waar moontlik, in verband gebring met die resultate van gestandardiseerde taaltoetse en aspekte in diegevalsgeskiendenis soos kultuur en die tydsverloop sedert die aanvang van die insident. die kei is weer na 'n periode van ses maande op 8 van die pasiente toegepas, om sodoende die sensitiwiteit vir herstel te evalueer. resultate dui aan dat die kei toepasbaar is op verskillende taalgroepe, en dat dit sensitief is vir herstel sowel as die kommunikasie-probleme as gevolg van beide linker en regterhemisfeerskade. dit korreleer goed met die algemene graad van die probleem. die kei onderskei egter nie pasiente ten opsigte van die tydsverloop sedert die aanvang van die insident nie. die potensielegebruik van die kei as 'n relatiefkultuurvrye evaluasie-instrument in die suid afrikaanse konteks, is bespreek. ι certain characteristics of the south african stroke population may be identified as being unique. these characteristics present a challenge to both the clinician and researcher, and often make it difficult and inappropriate to adapt some of the traditional approaches to assessment and treatment to local patients. the first factor to consider is that of multilingualism. the majority of aphasic patients in this country are bior multilingual (penn & beecham, 1992). most patients however are tested and treated in their second or third languages as most clinicians are currently unilingual. in addition despite several informal attempts (eg. semela, 1978) there have been no properly standardised versions of tests developed for aphasia. probably of even more importance, however, is the fact that the tests are not culture-free. even for english speaking south african aphasic patients, many of the test items of the aphasia tests currently in use are inappropriate for cultural reasons. another difficulty lies in the type of facilities available for the treatment of aphasia. despite the fact that the causes of aphasia, such as stroke, are amongst the highest in the world in the south african population, the facilities for treatment and rehabilitation are very inadequate (fritz & penn, 1992). the amount of time spent in an acute care hospital is much shorter than in other countries. in addition, there are (with very few exceptions) no rehabilitation hospitals or facilities available to the patient on discharge. within the hospital setting, very few patients have access to a range of rehabilitative therapies, except in urban areas. given that there are many vacant and unfilled posts in state health institutions, it is rare that an aphasic patient in any setting other than an urban one, will be seen by a speech-language therapist. the fact that there are unrealistic medical aid limits, exacerbates the problem, so that most patients after discharge are not able to afford private therapy rates. certain sociopolitical factors also have their impact on aphasia rehabilitation. due to a long history of inequality and educational disadvantage, the majority of aphasic patients in south africa are economically disadvantaged. many patients reside a long way from available facilities, and geographical distance and lack of transport are factors to be borne in mind. further, many patients are poorly educated, either functionally illiterate or barely literate, making the application of for® saslha 1992 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c o t s a n u n i c a t i v e effectiveness index: lised tests inappropriate. finally, high levels of unemploy1 1 1 3 jjj-eak up of family structure, no access to insurance etc, ke for a very mobile and inaccessible aphasic population. m a t h e aphasia clinician, working in a large hospital can norlly e x p e c t to see the patient during the acute phase only. after discharge, unless the patient is resident near the hospital d rhobile, it is unlikely that there will be follow up. the burden of rehabilitation thus often lies on the caregivers and those working in the community. for these reasons, there is an urgent need to re-address s o m e o f the basic tenets in aphasia. there has been a move in recent years towards a societal view of aphasia. this view hinges on the observation that a p h a s i c patients often have many communicative strengths which enable them to function relatively well in a social context, in contrast to their performance on traditional language measures (aten, 1986; goldblum, 1985; holland, 1980; penn, 1985). this shift in approach is reflected not only in the assessment measures used for aphasia, but also in increased acknowledgment and awareness of the importance of the context of the aphasic patient and on the estimation of the perceived effect of the disability on others in the environment. for example, some recent research has compared spouse and clinician ratings of aphasia. a study conducted in 1976 by helmick, watamori and palmer compared spouse and clinician ratings on the functional communication profile (sarno, 1975). they found considerable differences between spouse and clinician ratings, with spouses tending to underestimate the severity of the aphasia. this, it was suggested could lead to unrealistic expectations of the spouse. muller and code (1983) have also indicated that both spouse and aphasic persons are significantly more hopeful about eventual outcome than aphasiologists. holland (19 77 as cited by zraick & boone, 1991) on the other hand suggests that such a finding relates to the clinical biases of the speech therapists in evaluating aphasia. shewan and cameron (1984) found that in many instances spouses used in their study were unaware of aphasic-spouses' difficulties, although a better understanding of problems was noted in the groups of subjects receiving treatment than those that were not currently enrolled in speechtherapy. interestingly no difference in spouse patient agreement across severity was noted. j much of the disagreement among researchers may relate to differing methodologies of the studies as well as to the type of tool used to measure the impact of aphasia. it is of note, further, that although in recent years patients with right hemisphere lesions have been treated by speech language therapists and have been identified as having considerable social and pragmatic difficulties, no research on spouse attitudes appears to have been undertaken. a tool which has been developed recently which holds the promise of becoming a useful measure of spouse attitude both for aphasic and right hemisphere patients is the communicative effectiveness index (ceti) (lomas et al., 1989). the communicative effectiveness index (ceti) was developed in canada by a group of researchers interested in devising a relevant measure of functional communication for aphasic patients. first described by lomas et al.,, 1989, the index was drawn up on the basis of interviews with groups of aphasic patients and their spouses. a series of communication situations was generated from these interviews and through applications to groups of stable and acute patients, a final index of 16 situations was derived (see appendix 1). the ceti is administered to the spouse (or significant other) of the patients and involves the use of the visual analogue scale (vas). 1 its use with south african stroke patients 75 the results of the preliminary study by lomas et al. (1989) suggested that the ceti was both a valid and reliable tool as a measure of communication and an index of improvement as a result of treatment and/ or spontaneous recovery. however, lomas et al. called for additional research using this tool. furthermore, the authors pointed out that the ceti is not meant to be a substitute for standard testing, nor a comprehensive test of language functioning. neither is the ceti meant to discriminate between types of aphasia or act as a predictive tool. it does, however, reflect the severity which the effects of the aphasia have on communicative effectiveness, as perceived by a person in the environment who is likely to experience these effects on an everyday basis. the potential of the ceti as a tool for use in the aphasic population of south africa seems particularly good for a number of reasons. firstly it is based on interviews with aphasic patients and spouses and relates to the communication categories of basic need, health threat, life skill and social skill. as such, these items seem to be less sensitive to cultural and linguistic aspects, to literacy level and to socioeconomic status than many of the other tests/measures currently available. it also appears to be sensitive to the communication problems found in the right hemisphere stroke population. secondly it is administered to the spouse or caregiver of the patients and is concerned with everyday functional language behaviours. it thus focuses on a societal view of aphasia and identifies areas of strength and weakness relevant to those in the environment of the aphasic patient. for a number of reasons which are outlined above, in the majority of cases, rehabilitation and recovery of function of the aphasic patient is more likely to take place in a home setting, in the context of the family rather than in a coordinated rehabilitation setting. the attitude of the caregivers thus becomes a particularly important element in any consideration of aphasia. thirdly, the method of adminstration is simple, flexible, short and versatile, involving the use of the visual analogue scale which appears not to be dependent on literacy, numeracy level or education of the patient and the family. it is applicable in both the acute and relatively stable/chronic phases of aphasia. the purpose of the study was therefore to explore the use of the ceti within a south african context, in the hope that it will prove a culturally relevant index for the south african aphasic population by being flexible, socially valid and providing the clinician with some therapy guidelines as well as being amenable to a broad community based approach to rehabilitation. method the objectives of the study were as follows: to translate the ceti into sotho, zulu and afrikaans to administer it to a range of aphasic patients in each of four language groups (english, afrikaans, zulu, sotho) to administer it to a group of patients with right hemisphere damage to compare, where possible, results of ceti to the results of standardised language testing to examine the relationship between scores obtained on the ceti and overall severity of aphasia to examine the relationship between performance on the ceti and certain case history factors, eg.time since onset of the problem and racial/cultural background to readminister the ceti, where possible, after a period of time to assess its sensitivity to recovery and/or improvement following therapy to determine whether there is any characteristic item differentiation between groups. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 claire pen, kelly milner, peter fridjhon it was hypothesised that: 1. the nature of the aphasic deficit in a group of south african aphasic patients would be highlighted by means of the ceti. 2. there would be a difference between the scores of patients tested on the ceti after a period of recovery. 3. the patients with right hemisphere damage may show a communicative deficit as highlighted by the ceti. 4. there would be some correlation between the type of standardised test profile that the patient displays and the results on the ceti. subjects a multicultural group of 28 adult stroke patients was used in this study. the subjects were divided into three groups: group a. 14 subjects were diagnosed as aphasic patients in that they had a single episode, cerebral vascular accident of sudden onset and had left hemisphere (lh) damage in the language areas, as confirmed by the report of a neurologist and /or speech language pathologist. this group of patients had a single ceti administered. the mean age of this group was 57 years and the mean time since onset of aphasia was 14 months. group b. an additional 8 aphasic subjects, who fulfilled the same criteria as patients in group a were tested on two occasions (designated b1 and b2). four members of this group were stable (ie. first tested at more than six months post onset) and four were acute or "recovering" (to use the terminology of lomas et al.) that is less than six months post onset. the purpose of this was to allow for a comparison with the data of lomas et al. (1989). the mean number of months between first and second testing for these two groups was 3 months for the acute group and 13 months for the stable group respectively. the mean age of this group was 56 years. group c. the remaining six subjects had a communication problem resulting from a right hemisphere (rh) stroke, as determined by the report of a neurologist. these patients were included in the study because the communication effects of right hemisphere damage have been well documented and are sensitive to societal measures of performance such as the ceti. the mean age of these patients was 52.5 years and the mean time since onset was 4 months. for various reasons outlined in the introduction, the selection of a homogenous group of aphasic patients is particularly difficult in the south african context. criteria for subject selection were thus restricted to the following: each subject should have a spouse and/or significant other spending at least three days a week with him/her, (in order to ensure accuracy in filling out the ceti) and each subject should have as a home language one of the following four languages: english, afrikaans, zulu or sotho (as the measure was translated in these languages only). other factors such as age, gender, educational level, time since onset of the problem and previous therapy were not controlled for. details pertaining to the subjects are portrayed in table 1. .the age rangeofthe subjects wasfrom 19-82 years. 18english speaking patients, 3 afrikaans-speaking patients, 6 zulu speaking patients and 3 sotho patients were used in the study. information pertaining to the spouse or significant other who completed the ceti is also provided in table 1. patients were identified by speech therapy departments and clinics in the johannesburg areas and were tested over a period of two years. method 1. translation of the ceti. the canadian version of the ceti was translated verbatim into three languages: afrikaans, sotho and zulu. these languages were selected on the basis of their being most commonly used by patients in the area of johannesburg. translations were undertaken by first-language qualified speech-language and hearing therapists in the appropriate home language and were checked for accuracy by an additional native speaker and by the department of african languages at the university of the witwatersrand. none of the conten t of the ceti was altered because the authors of the test (lomas et al., 1989) have suggested that the items have universal application. 2. administration of the ceti. the ceti was administered to all the subjects in the following way: the authors provided a broad background to graduate or student therapists who were working with the particular patient. student therapists were instructed on the use and administration of the ceti in accordance with the directions set out by lomas et al. (1989). the spouse or significant other was interviewed and informed of the purpose of the study. the ceti was then completed, following a brief discussion of the vas with the spouse/caregiver. 3. additional testing. in addition, formalised testing of the patients took place. in most cases a standard aphasia test (eg. the boston diagnostic aphasia examination (bdae), goodglass and kaplan 1972; the western aphasia battery (wab), kertesz 1982) or translated versions thereof were administered over one or two test periods with the patients. the procedure followed the designated instructions in the manual. formalised testing was not always possible, however, for the following reasons: the patient was still in the acute phase and therefore difficult to test, a translated version of the standard test was not available or the patient had been discharged before standardised testing could be undertaken. in such cases, an informal judgement of type and severity of aphasia was made on the basis of unstandardised assessment tasks (eg. a language sample) and discussion with the therapists of the patients. where sufficient data were available a severity rating, using the severity rating scale of the boston diagnostic aphasia examination (goodglass & kaplan, 1980) was also undertaken. the purpose of this testing was to provide some baseline of comparison for the results obtaine'd on the ceti. ί 4. follow up testing. for the eight lh patients whom it was possible to follow up, a ceti was readministered (mean time interval 6 months; range 3 months 15 months). this time period was selected in order to ensure, as far as possible, that the period of spontaneous recovery was past (kertesz, 1982). follow-up testing was done on a previously marked ceti as lomas et al. suggest, giving the spouses the benefit of seeing where their previous judgement had been (guyatt, borman, townsend & taylor 1985). the time interval between the two testing sessions for the repeat group was generally longer than in the case of the lomas et al. (1989) study. scoring method and analysis of data the scoring of the ceti involves the use of-'the visual analogue scale. not only is it assumed to be particularly sensitive to changes in performance over time as bond and lader (1974) have suggested, it is also considered relevant for the type of population used in this study as jacobson (1986) and others have shown that it is not reliant on literacy and numeracy levels. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39. 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communicative effectiveness index: its use with south african stroke patients table 1: subject characteristics 77 ss sex age time since onset first language result of std testing severtiy therapy person interviewed group a aphasic subjects, single testing 1 μ 56 3y;2m zulu expressive severe y wife 2 f 46 -;2m zulu wab:aq 12.5 severe y niirse-aid 3 μ 64 -;4m soth wab:aq 76.8 mild y son 4 μ 41 -;3m eng bdae:1 severe y wife 5 f 51 5y eng wab:aq 57 severe y husband 6 μ 24 ly;4m eng cadl: anomic mild y father 7 f 82 -;lm eng wab:aq 90.4 mild y daughter 8 μ 59 -,8m, eng bdae:1 severe y wife 9 f 70 3y eng wab:aq 85.7 mild y husband 10 μ 47 l y eng wab:aq 92.6 mild y wife 11 μ 63 -,8m afr wab:aq 68 y daughter 12 f 78 -;lm eng wab:aq 45 ν husband 13 μ 58 eng wab:aq 77 mild y son 14 μ 61 3y eng bdae: 2 severe y wife group β aphasic patients with repeat testing 15 f 59 ly;3m eng wab:aq 37.9 severe y husband 16 f 57 -;3m afr wab:aq 46.7 mild y son 17 f 57 6 y eng wab:aq 65.6 mild y husband 18 μ 72 3y eng y wife 19 μ 52 -;lm zulu wab:aq 82.8 mild y brother in law 20 μ j65 -;5m soth wab:aq 12.3 severe y son / 21 μ 53 -;2m zulu wab:aq 56.7 severe ν wife 22 f 36 -;9m zulu bdae:3 mild y mother 1 group c right hemisphere group ! 23 f j 60 —4m soth wab:aq 89.5 mild y wife 24 μ ' 6 1 / -;3m eng wab:aq 89.4 mild ν 25 μ 67 -;lm eng wab:aq 79.4 cousin 26 f 69 -;lm eng severe sister 27 f 39 —4m zulu wab:aq 92.8 mild y mother 28 f 19 -;9m , afr wab:aq 79.9 mild y niece key and explanation to table 1 -: information not available first language: english (eng), sotho (soth), zulu or afrikaans (afr) aphasia type/score: determined by formal testing using bdae or wab where possible, otherwise by informal observation and/or therapist report. wab aqs reported when available. sev: severity as rated on the severity rating scale of the boston diagnostic aphasia examination (goodglass & kaplan, 1980) prev ther: history of previous therapy (y: yes; n: no) other: significant other interviewed th south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 78 claire penn, kellymilner, peter fridjhon the position on the scale of each response was calculated by measurement on a 100 millimeter scale and an overall percentage score was thus obtained for each item and for each patient, moved to full decimal point. scores for each item were calculated and means for each item across the three subjectgroups were calculated. statistical testing as the sample sizes were small and the normality of the population questionable, non parametric measures were used to test group differences namely the mann whitney u test and the wilcoxon matched pairs signed-rank test (siegel, 1965). group differences were tested between groups a, β and c, between group a and β combined (representing the lh group) versus group c, and between the two testing periods of group β (referred to as b1 and b2). statistical testing was also undertaken for the variables of severity, time since onset and linguistic/cultural background, with the lh group (groups a and β combined).this was done in order to relate the subjects' performance on the ceti to case history factors known to relate to aphasia test performance. severity. the overall severity of the aphasia, as measured on a standard test or on the boston diagnostic aphasia examination severity rating scale (goodglass & kaplan, 1980), was characterised and patients divided into mild versus severe aphasic patients (ie. for groups a and b). 10 patients were classified as mild and 9 as severe patients on this scale. the severity level of three of the patients was not possible to assess either because of missing data, or because their classification was equivocal (eg. in the case of conduction aphasia). time since onset. the cut off point of six months, which is the period traditionally considered as reflecting a period of spontaneous recovery, was selected (kertesz, 1983). in groups a and β combined (that is the lh subjects),ten subjects had a time since onset of less than six months and 11 subjects a time since onset of more than six months. linguistic/cultural background. aphasic patients (that is groups a and b) were split into two groups reflecting their relative racial/cultural background. for the purposes of this study the groups used were white (including english and afrikaans speakers) and black (including sotho and zulu speakers). the numbers were 7 (black) and 15 (white) respectively. this split, though acknowledged to be crude, reflects in some way the split created by sociopolitical divisions and rehabilitation services offered in this country to date. an analysis of differences by language was also done in order to ensure that the translation of the ceti did not affect the results obtained. item analysis. in order to gain some insight into the specific nature of the communication problems experienced by the subjects in this study, an item analysis was undertaken, using a measure splitting the scores obtained into quartiles. as there were no scores in the bottom quartiles, the first and second quartiles were combined into one catogory, hence deriving the following three ranges: 0-49, 50-74 and 75 -100. this was done by means of a matrix (see appendix 2) whereby items that were not consistent across groups were identified. (inconsistent items were off the major diagonal of the matrix). this analysis was undertaken for the aphasia group as a whole, and for comparing the two testing sessions for group b. it was also undertaken for the rh group as well as for the variables of severity, time since onset, and culture, using the aphasic patients (that is groups a and b). sample size prohibited such a breakdown in the case of the rh group (group c). results and discussion of results results of statistical testing a. group scores on the ceti the means of the subjects' scores in each group for each item are presented in table 2. this table reveals the following trends: 1. no significant difference exists between the overall scores for groups α, β1 and c. this confirms that the ceti not only seems sensitive to south african aphasic disorders but also to the problems following right hemisphere damage. the fact that no differences existed between the scores of groups a and β justifies their combination in certain of the analyses undertaken. for group a (lh) a mean score (across all items) was 58.2% with a range of 35 79 %. for group β (testing period 1) the mean score was 59% (range 35-77%) and for group c the mean was 70% ( range 40 86%). this was done using the mann whitney u test (between a and β1 u=k; ns; between a and c u=k; ns). 2. for group b, there was a significant difference between the scores obtained for the two testing periods (59% versus 70% respectively) with a mean improvement of 10.6% across time (wilcoxon matched pairs testing, t=14, p=.05). this was true for both the stable group and the acute group (the mean difference in improvement being 12.5 percent and 8.8 percent respectively). this difference was found to be significant (mann whitney u test; u=5; p=.05) suggesting that both groups showed improvement on the second testing. the results are, however, somewhat problematic because of the small sample size of the two groups and because of the unexpected results in one of the stable patients (subject 22) who made a very large improvement (32%) from first to second testing arid therefore skewed results for the stable group. | these findings confirm the results of lomas et al. (1989) and suggest that the ceti is sensitive to changes across time. not all patients in this group showed a positive change across time however; two subjects' scores went down (subjects 15 table 2: mean scores per group across ceti items ceti item 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 total .. group a 68 59 72' 63 79 69 61 65 62 51 55 36 66 37 44 35 58 group b1 66 50 59 64 75 56 59 56 77 55 68 44 67 35 52 53 ' 5 9 group b2 82 64 79 71 84 73 75 67 94 67 81 50 61 44 62 70' 70 group c 55 63 86 56 64 53 67 60 73 51 70 52 52 40 56 ' 51 70 (scores are given in percentages) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c o u n i c a t i v e effectiveness index:its use with south african stroke patients 79 and 20 by about 1.1 and 5% respectively). looking at the individual subject profiles of these two patients, they were both severe and both were receiving speech-language therapy at the time of the second testing. thus the negative change in ceti scores might be accounted for in terms of more realistic attitudes having been developed by the spouses. as discussed earlier, research has indicated that spouses whose partners are enrolled in therapy tend to become increasingly aware of the aphasic individual's difficulties (shewan & cameron, 1984). what is very interesting to note here, is that there is a change in the stable patients, despite claims made that a plateau is reached after a period of time. changes in long term patients do however persist (eg sarno, 1975; aten, 1986) particulary if these changes are measured on functionally relevant test measures. it is therefore not surprising that while the four chronic patients had no significant changes on standard test scores over time, they did reveal differences on the cetibecoming, perhaps, more "experienced" aphasic persons. 3. the mean score for the right hemisphere group (group c) was 60.1% confirming that the right hemisphere patient does have substantial communication deficits frequently not detected by traditional tests (myers, 1984; sherratt & penn, 1990). interestingly the extent of the deficit in the rh group is not significantly different from the lh groups (mann whitney test), confirming the severe societal implication that such a defect has for the patient and the need for communication therapy for right-hemisphere brain-damaged subjects. b. analysis of aphasia patients in terms of subject characteristics the results of the analysis undertaken using aphasic subjects from groups a and β in relation to the variables of severity, time since onset and culture are as follows: 1. severity. a significant difference in the ceti score was obtained between patients who were classified as mild aphasia (with a mean of 70%) and those classified as severe (mean of 51%) (mann whitney u; u=15; p<.05). this finding supports the results of lomas et al. (1989) in sugggesting that the ceti has some validity in that it correlates with other broad measures of severity and therefore has the potential to discriminate accurately between different levels of patients. 2. time since onset. ten subjects had a time since onset of less than six months and 11 subjects had a time since onset of more than six months (data on one subject was missing). there was no significant difference found between misan scores of these two groups (u=k; ns). once again this may indicate that spouses become more aware of the difficulties faced by the aphasic person with time.jthus the plight of the acutely aphasic individual may be under-'emphasized, while the chronic patient may be viewed more realistically. 3. cultural and language aspects. there was no significant difference found between ceti scores across any of the linguistic groups (english, afrikaans, sotho and zulu) or the two cultural groups (7 black and 15 white subjects) in terms of overall ceti means in groups a and b1 (u=k; ns). this confirms the hypothesis that the ceti is a relatively culture-free assessment tool, tapping areas of universal relevance to aphasic patients. it also suggests that the translation of the ceti did not appear to affect its reliability . c. item analysis (refer appendix 2) 1. item analysis for aphasic patients. the following items having a consistent level of severe difficulty across each of the aphasia groups (group a and b): item 12 (starting a conversation with people who are not close family), item 14 (being part of conversation when it is fast and there is a number of people involved), item 15 (participating in a conversation with strangers) and item 16 (describing or discussing something in depth). of moderate difficulty were: item 8 (saying the name of someone whose face is in front of him/her) and item 7 (having one-to-one conversation with spouse). of least difficulty to the groups was item 3 (giving yes and no answers appropriately). such an analysis confirms that communication situations with high social and cognitive demands are perceived as more impaired after a stroke than others. such findings also provide directions for intervention in terms of a possible continuum of management contexts. 2. item analysis differences in retested patients (appendix 2.2). the differences between items for patients in group β over the two testing periods was analysed. the assumption underlying this analysis was that certain aspects of communicative behaviour may show a particular sensitivity to change over time. no items deteriorated but interestingly, four items showed an improvement across time in group β namely: item 1: (getting someone's attention), item 3: (giving yes/no answers appropriately). item 11: (responding or communicating anything without words) and item 12: (starting a conversation with people who are not close family). the differences in item difficulty between b1 and b2 confirm the modality effect for spontaneous recovery described by lomas and kertesz (1978), that is that certain modalities (for example comprehension) improve at different rates from others and demonstrate that as the individual interacts with persons of the environment, s/he becomes more functional in the way that s/ he uses language (penn, 1983; aten, 1986 ). 3. item analysis for the right hemisphere group (refer appendix 2.1). the profile of the rh group was very different in terms of item complexity from the profile of the lh groups. for example, item 15 (starting a conversation with strangers), which presented the aphasic group with severe difficulties did not seem to be too taxing for the rh patients . this is a strong confirmation of the literature which documents the rh patients' relative lack of sensitivity to the environment and to the social situation (millar &whitaker, 1983; myers, 1984). in contrast, understanding writing (item 13) appeared less difficult for the aphasic patients than for the rh patients, suggesting that the visuo-spatial deficits of the rh patients may well be interfering substantially with the communicative process. 4. item analysis by severity (appendix 2.4). there were several items which differentiated the mild from the severe aphasic patient; namely items 8, 9, 10, 12, 13, 15 and 16. of particular significance was item 8 saying the name of someone whose face is in front of him/her. this item provided maximum differentiation between the mild and the severe aphasic groups, highlighting that it is in the area of lexical specificity that the aphasic individual's language is typically impaired. anomia is well documented as a primary symptom of the aphasia deficit and it is interesting that it is this item, on a functional measure such as the ceti, that should differentiate mild from severe patients so clearly. 5. item analysis by time since onset (appendix 2.5). there was a surprising finding that many items were rated by spouses as being more difficult in the group who were tested more than six months post onset (ν 12) compared to those who were tested before this cut-off point (n9). examples of such items include 10 and 14 , (involving conversational skills), 9 (communicating physical problems) and 13 (understanding writing). this finding requires some explanation. it is hypothe south african journal of communication disorders, vol 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 80 claire penn, kelly milner, peter fridjhon thesised that the spouse may have become more realistic about the aphasic patient's problems with time. based on the results of the subjects in group b, this is not to say that they will not improve across time, but the tendency for early stage aphasic patients to be overrated in terms of their abilities has been documented previously (helmick et al., 1976; muller & code, 1983) and is confirmed by these results. the difficulties involved in conversing spontaneously, being involved in group situations and in understanding writing may well only emerge as the patient attempts to re-enter pre-morbid social and/or occupational arenas. 6. item analysis by culture (appendix 2.3). the seven black patients in groups a and β were considered for separate analysis to determine whether any differences emerged when compared to the group of white patients. the results of this analysis suggest that overall performance on the ceti is similar for both black and white subjects. on item 15, however, black patients performed better than white (participating in a conversation with strangers) and white patients performed better than black on item 2 (getting involved in a group conversation) and on item 5 (indicating understanding of what is being said). it is not possible to offer an explanation for these findings at present, but they could be suggestive of possible cultural effects of items. it is suggested that these variables require validation through further research. general discussion the results of this study suggest the following: the ceti seems to be a useful tool in the context of south africa in that it was successfully applied to a multicultural group of south african aphasic patients and reveals results comparable to those found by the original authors of the test on a group of canadian patients. equivalent results were obtained for aphasic patients regardless of racial, cultural or linguistic background. further, it appears to be sensitive to effects of severity and to recovery aspects and also highlights the communication deficits remaining after right hemisphere stroke. explanations for some of these results seem worth exploring. the fact that the results concur with those of lomas et al. (1989) confirms the strong societal impact which aphasia has, regardless of culture, language and country of origin. though attitudes to disability may change from culture to culture, the essence of the aphasic deficit, that is communication disability, must surely be universal in its effect. the analysis procedure used in this study made possible an investigation of specific item difficulty both within and between groups. unfortunately lomas et al. (1989) have not reported the results of their item comparison. it appears however that the present analysis of individual items has yielded some interesting results which may well have therapeutic implications. the fact that the zulu and sotho speaking patients, for example, were found to be less affected for group conversation than their white counterparts reflects possible differences in cultural attitudes towards such a disability, as well as suggesting possible directions for the context of rehabilitation. this aspect should be explored in future studies, for if we are to provide adequate aphasia rehabilitation services to the vast black population of this country, it is imperative that we begin to develop some knowledge of the african understanding of such a disorder. the method of free interviews to generate items within different cultural contexts, would appear to be a most interesting and valuable area for further research. it should be remembered that the original items selected from a pool of 36 items in the lomas study were generated by only 14 canadian aphasic patients. there is therefore a strong possibility that some additional items may well be derived in a local study of this nature. another dimension for comparison with the lomas study lies in the results obtained when comparing the eight patients who were tested twice on the ceti. it should be noted that in the lomas et al (1989) study the ceti was readministered only six weeks after its first administration, and that an improvement was noted in the ceti score only for the recovering (acute) group of subjects, but not in the stable group (more than six months post-onset). however in the present study, (where the mean time interval between test periods was 13 months for the stable group and three months for the recovering group respectively) an improvement was seen in both groups. this finding highlights the need to measure the effect of the improvement over a longer period of time than six weeks. in south africa, few patients receive intensive therapy following the stroke, whereas in the lomas et al. study, it should be noted that recovering patients were more likely to have received intensive therapy. the findings that stable patients continued to improve, and in contrast to the lomas et al. study, showed a significant change with time, thus may be accounted for in terms of the different time period used in the studies. the case of subject 22 in group β is particularly interesting. a younger subject, she showed a large amount of change, despite the fact that the period traditionally defined as the period of spontaneous recovery had long passed. not only is the patient relatively young,, confirming the writing of lomas and kertesz (1978) and others, regarding rate and amount of recovery in younger stroke patients, but she had also received intensive therapy and support from those in her environment and it is felt that the results seen here reflect the value that such stimulation has, well beyond the period of plateau in language skills, in overall communicative effectiveness. the use of the term "recovering" by lomas et al (1989) to describe the patient whose neurological picture is unstable, is perhaps a misnomer, because in contrast to the lomas study even the stable patients in the present study appear to be "recovering". the results for the right hemisphere patients are particularly fascinating. the ceti seems to be sensitive to the communication problems that such patients are having. though there is no difference in overall score on the ceti for these patients and the aphasic patients, the item-analysis has highlighted specific problems in different areas. this certainly lends support to the notion that right hemisphere patients need to be referred for communication therapy even if their results on standard measures of language are not significant. this population is very often overlooked in referral to jthe speech and language therapist. nevertheless the societal and familial ramifications of this type of disorder may well! be amenable to appropriately directed therapy. for example item 5 (indicating understanding of what is being said), which proved to be more difficult for the rh than the lh groups, indicates a need for socially directed compensatory training in such patients. conclusions the results of the present study suggest that a measure such as the ceti appears to have some validity on a south african stroke population regardless of cultural and linguistic background. as this measure focuses on the societal and contextual ramifications of aphasia, it becomes a viable alternative to some of the approaches currently being used in assessment as well as providing some guidelines for intervention, not only the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) c o u n i c a t i v e effectiveness index:its use with south african stroke patients 81 with the aphasic patient but also for the patient with comm u n i c a t i o n problems resulting from right hemisphere damage. . the writers suggest that the ideal south african aphasia test should have the following properties: * it should be relatively culture free hence applicable to all aphasic patients in south africa, regardless of linguistic, cultural, social and educational backgrounds * it should be administered in the patient's home language, preferably by someone who is culturally and linguistically compatible with the patient * it should be easy to administer and not reliant on factors such as expensive equipment, tape recording of samples (because of the contextual restraint imposed by so many of the testing environments) * it should be relevant to the context (familial, social and occupational) of the patient * it should be sensitive to changes in the patient's status and thus be reflective of either spontaneous recovery or of therapy * it should have desired psychometric properties including reliability and validity in a preliminary way, it is hoped that this study involving the use of the ceti has shown that such a goal is within our reach. although no claims can be made about its being comprehensive, diagnostic or definitive, this approach clearly has promise, in that it appears to be both relevant and flexible for local patients. acknowledgements the writers wish to express sincere thanks to the following persons for their assistance during this study: dilys jones and beverley aron of the department of speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand ; the speech therapists at johannesburg, hillbrow and coronation hospitals; speech-therapy students: marion flink, precious jenga, tamar mehl and florence mthoba. this study was supported by a grant from the human sciences research council. reference list j aten, j. (1986). functional communication treatment. in r. chapey, (ed.), language intervention strategies in adult aphasia. new york: williams and wilkins. bond, a. & lader, m. (1974).the use of analogue scales in rating subjective feelings. british journal \of medical psychology, 47, 211 -218. fritz, v. & penn, c. (eds) (1992). stroke: caring and coping johannesburg witwatersrand university press. goldblum, g. (1985). aphasia: a societal and clinical appraisal of pragmatic and linguistic behaviours. south african journal of communication disorders 32, 11-17. goodglass, h. & kaplan, e.(1972). the assessment of aphasia and related disorders. philadelphia: lea and febiger. guyatt, g„ berman, l., townsend, m. & taylor, d. (1985). should study subjects see their previous respons e? journal of chronic diseases,38, 1003 1007. helmickj., waltamori, t. & palmer, j. (1976). spouses understanding of communication disabilities of aphasic patients jourwai of speech and hearing disorders, 41, 238 243. holland, a. l. (1980). communicative abilities in daily living: a test of junctional communication for aphasic adults. baltimore: university park press. jacobson, m. c. (1986). speech intelligibility and articulatory dynamics of reconstructive oral cancer patients. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. kertesz, a. (1979). aphasia and associated disorders. new york: grune and stratton. kertesz, a.(1980). western aphasia battery. new york: grune and stratton. lomas, j., pickard, l., beste, s., elbard, h., finlayson, a. &zoghaib, c. (1989). the communicative effectiveness index: development and psychometric evaluation of a functional communication measure for adult aphasia. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 54, 113 124. lomas, j. & kertesz, a. (1978). patterns of spontaneous recovery in aphasic groups: a study of adult stroke patients. brain and language, 5, 388 401. millar, j. & whitaker, h. (1983). the right hemisphere's contribution to language: a review of the evidence from brain damaged subjects. in s.j. segalowitz (ed.), language functions and brain organization. new york: academic press. muller, d. & code, c. (1983). psychosocial adjustment to aphasia. british journal of disorders of communication, 18, 23 29. myers, p. s. (1984). right hemisphere impairment. in a. holland (ed.) language disorders in adults. san diego: college hill. penn, c. (1985). the profile, of communicative appropriateness : a clinical tool for the assessment of pragmatics. south africanjournal of communication disorders, 32, 18 24. penn, c. & beecham, r. (1992). discourse therapy in multilingual aphasia: a case-study. clinical linguistics and phonetics, 6,(1), 11-25. sarno, μ. t. (1975). the functional communication profile. new york: institute of rehabilitative medicine. semela, j.j. (1978). an investigation into the breakdown of the concordial system of zulu-speaking aphasics. unpublished undergraduate research project, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. shewan, c. & cameron, h. (1984). communication and related problems as perceived by aphasic individuals and their spouses. journal of communication disorders, 17, 175 -187. sherratt, s. & penn, c. (1990). discourse in a right-hemisphere braindamaged subject. aphasiology, 4, (6), 539 560. siegel, s (1965). nonparametric statistics for the behavioral sriences. new york: mcgraw-hill. zraik, r. & boone, d. (1991). spouse atitudes towards the person with aphasia. journal of speech and hearing research, 34, 123 128. appendix 1: communication situations used in the communicative effectiveness index ceti items 1. getting someone's attention 2. getting involved in group conversations that are about him/her. 3. giving yes/no answers appropriately. , 4. communicating his/her emotions. 5. indicating that he/she understands what is being said to him/her. 6. having coffee time visits and conversations with friends and neighbours 7. having a one-to-one conversation with you. 8. saying the name of someone whose face is in front of him/ her. 9. communicating physical problems such as aches and pains. 10. having a spontaneous conversation. 11. responding or communicating anything without words. 12. starting a conversation with people who are not close family. 13. understanding writing. 14. being part of a conversation when it is fast and there are a number of people involved 15. participating in a conversation with strangers. 16. describing or discussing something in depth. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 claire penn, kelly milner, peter fridjhon appendix 2: item comparison matrices (the numbers refer to item numbers of the ceti.) 2.1: comparison of item difficulty for lh (group a ) versus r h (group b ) patients group a mean score <49% 50-74% 75%+ <49% 14 12 group c 50 74% 15; 16 1; 2; 4; 6; 7; 8; 9 10; 11; 13; 5; 75+% 3; 2.2 comparison of item difficulty on repeat testing for group β black subjects <49% 50-74% 75+% <49% 14 b2 50 74% 12 2; 4; 6; 7 8; 13 15; 16 75+% 1; 3; 11 5; 9; 10 2.3 comparison of item difficulty for black vs white subjects black subjects <49% 50-74% 75+% <49% 12; 14; 16 15; white subjects 50 74% 2; 1; 3; 4; 6 7; 8; 9; 10 11; 13 75+% 5 2.4 comparison of item difficulty for severe vs mild patients. severley aphasic subjects ! <49% 50-74% 75+ ! <49% 14; 1 i mildly aphasic subjects 50 74% 2; 10; 12; 15 16; 1; 3; 4; 6; 7; 11 1 75+% 8 9; 18 5 2.5: comparison of itemdifficulty for subjects tested in stable versus recovering subjects < 6m <49% 50-74% 75+% <49% 12; 16; 10; 14; 15 > 6m 50 74% 5; 1; 2; 3; 4; 6; 7; 8; 11 / 9; 13; 75+% the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ® in-the-canal hearing aids with acoustimed's exclusive "anti-feedback" technology unlimited venting without whistling more usable gain than behind-the-ear aids user-controlled noise reduction easily handled battery drawer the acoustic advantages of in-the-canal hearing aids are well known. however, until now their use has been limited by acoustic feedback which causes oscillation (whistling). acoustimed has developed a technique for controlling the phase response of hearing aids to prevent oscillation. full details are a trade secret but results are spectacular. we can now make in-the-canal hearing aids that have more usable gain than behind-the-ear models and they do not oscillate regardless of how large we make the vent. with the phantom model afp (power) version it is possible to correct nerve deafness with high frequency loss as great as 90db. the phantom models af and afp have a simple but effective noise control. when the volume is turned down low frequency noise is reduced more than the essential high frequencies. this unique feature'has the effect of improving the signal-to-noise ratio in noisy conditions. because of the miicrophone position phantom hearing aids have better high frequency performance than since there is no efficient and hav'e behind-the-ear models and tubing they are more a smoother frequency response. previously in-the-canal hearing aids could be used only for mild hearing loss due to feedback problems. with the new phantom hearing' aids there are less feedback problems than with behind-the-ear models. tnis allows us to make phantom aids which work better than many behind-the-ear pp hearing aids. phantom hearing aids are made in 3 basic models. the approximate fitting range is illustrated in tnis diagram. performance is controlled by transducer selection. frequency 125 250 500 ik 2k akhz acoustimed hearing services 327 bosman building cor. eloff and bree streets, johannesburg tel: (311) 337-2977 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) information for contributors theoretical and philosophical mnce^uiahsi a n d c n t ? c a " y evaluative of human communication « t s η 6 a h n g w i t h a s p e c t s ing; and p o l i c y . " d ' t s d l s o r d e r s > s e ™ c e provision; trainunder review hy other ̂ t t ^ ^ ° γ * * i s manuscript style and requirements quired to follow strictly he s^le z ™ ^ i c o n t r i b ^ ° n s are rewith complete internal consistent p „ „ , ' p u b · m a n · ) , quality type-written m a n z c ^ z t n t ^ 0 t n p l e " s p a c e d high margins should be submitted ̂ tiev^hoi p a « e s · -nd wide identical disc copies o 7 £ p a w l ) t n w τ ™ 0 extension ,wp5); (2) in a s a ^ n l v l n 1 w o r d p e r f e « 5.1 (with an names shouzin ludetheirst a u t w ^ ' t ? e x t 6 n s i o n a s c ) · able keyword or abbreviatio^w f t f a " d 3 d e a r ' y i d e n t i f i " the last line of t h e a * p a g e o f x % / ^ * ^ e w r i « e n on purposes only). p g ° f t h e r e f e r e n c e list (for retrieval warranted. the fllpage of twozttil ^ a d d l t l ° n a l k n g t h i s article, name of authorfs) ^ ^ contain the title of the accordance address).in not required to p ^ d ^ ^ 1 ^ f h o r s are copies, the first page should contain oriy tĥ tirie s t τ * all copies, should contain only a n a b s ^ « h nn f of english and a f r i k a a n s afrit.. a b s t r a c t (100 words), written in seas contributor·! mo,• raans abstracts will be provided for overorder of m ^ o t s s m ^ p l i m b l e s h o u l d * in the a c k n o w i ^ d g e ^ s s g 1 ^ conclusion, be indented. "^tjumvces. all paragraphs should s ™ e t p s u s r u , d w i e c c h s hrf l d b e « « karate the copies sem ε ί ^ ρ ΐ ^ γ η ι τ γ p u r p 0 s e s a " d u s e d f o r p u b h c a t i o „ h o w e v f " m u s t s , rtan , h n e d r a w i n g s t h a t a r e references after the first o c c u « ^ « λ * * τ ^ m ° t e t h a n ^thors, for six or more wherf«a/ m v· author will suffice, except authors shouldtppeati^tl^e referenr^r°^ n a m s °f all strict alphabetical order i s s ® ' ̂ s h ° u i d b e i i s t e d » references should betnc udx f d °f the artide· a " ces, (αρα pub. man. l983 p l 3 ) o n t ' ' " ^ ΐ , " ® journals may be used, (se« d s h ^ o t m c ^ ^ n t v. k r e v * a t * o n s ° f list of scientific periodicals). °t t^ w o r l d exceed much more than 25, unless z x l n y t ? ^ ^ n « examples locke, j.l. (1983). c ] i n i c a ] h o l 3m4erof s p e e c h s o u n d wilkins. m 1 d e d j b a ]t«nore: williams & editing ^ ^ ι τ ^ ^ η ι ^ " γ 6 d t 0 for revision. required. the i t " m a d e * t h e is fina. editing f o r s ^ ^ : ^ ^ · ^ ^ * 10 reprints with°ut — -11 ̂ provided free of s z ^ ^ s z s z s r t h e pre t o d 4» " arrangement. p p b e a c c e p t e d until 31s't july by ι queries, correspondence & manuscript i ., , s i r s box 31782, b r a ' a m l ^ ^ t o ^ r u k i ^ r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) inligting vir bydraers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikadeafitykings publiseer verslae en artikels wat gemoeid is met navorsing, of handel oor krities evaluerende, teoretiese en filosofiese konseptuele kwessies wat oor menslike kommunikasie en kommunikasieafwykings; diensverskaffing; opleiding en beleid gaan. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafivykings sal nie artikels aanvaar wat reeds elders gepubliseer is, of wat tans deur ander publikasies oorweeg word nie. manuskrip styl en vereistes: manuskripte behoort deur 'n dekkings brief vergesel te word wat die skrywer se adres en telefoonnommers bevat. daar word van alle bydraes verwag om die styl, soos gespesifiseer is in die "publication manual ofthe american psychological assoc. (3rd ed., 1983) (αρα pub. man."), nougeset te volg met volledige interne ooreenstemming. manuskripte moet getik, van hoe gehalte en in drievoud spasiering met wye kantlyne wees. vier kopiee van die manuskrip moet verskaf word. twee hiervan moet identiese skyfkopiee van die artikel wees; (1) in "wordperfect" 5.1 (met 'n uitbreiding .wp5); (2) in ascii kodes met 'n uitbreiding .asc). leername behoort die eerste skrywer se voorletters en 'n duidelike identifiseerbare sleutelwoord of afkorting daarvan in te sluit en moet op die laaste lyn van die laaste bladsy van die verwysingslys getik word (slegs vir naslaan doeleindes). as 'n reel moet bydraes nie 25 bladsye oorskry nie, maar langer artikels sal aanvaar word indien die addisionele lengte dit regverdig. op die eerste bladsy van twee van die afskrifte moet die titel van die artikel, naam van die skrywers(s), en instansie (of adres) verskyn. in ooreenstemming met die "αρα pub. man." se styl word daar nie van skrywers verwag om enige kwalifikasies te verskaf nie. op die eerste bladsy van die twee oorblywende afskrifte moet slegs die titel van die artikel verskaf word. die tweede bladsy van alle afskrifte moet slegs 'n opsomming (100 woorde) in beide engels en afrikaans bevat. afrikaanse opsommings sal vir buitelandse bydraers voorsien word. hoofopskrifte moet, waar van toepassing, in die volgende volgorde verskaf word: metode, resultate, bespreking, gevolgtrekking, erkenn1ngs en verwysings. alle paragrawe moet ingekeep word. ! tabelle en figure wat op afsonderlike bladsye (een bladsy per tabel/illustrasie) moet verskynj moet vir referent doeleindes gekopieer word en slegs die kopiee moet jinisieel verskaf word. figure, grafieke en lyntekeninge wat vir publikasie gebruik word, moet egter oorspronklike weergawes wees en moet in swart ink op wit papier van 'n hoe gehalte wees. die oorspronklikes sal slegs verlang word nadat die artikel vir publikasie aanvaar is. letterwerk wat op bogenoemde verskyn moet eenvormig wees, professioneel gedoen word en daar moet in gedagte gehou word dat dit leesbaar moet wees na 'n 50% verkleining in drukwerk. letterwerk by illustrasies moet onder geen omstandighede getik word nie. verklarings of legendes moet nie in die illustrasie nie, maar daaronder, verskyn. die opskrifte van tabelle (wat boaan verskyn), en die onderskrifte van figure, (wat onderaan verskyn), moet beknop, maar helderend wees. numering moet deur middel van arabiese syfers geskied. tabelle en figure moet in die volgorde waarin hulle verskyn, genommer word. die aantal tabelle en illustrasies wat ingesluit word, word deur die redakteur bepaal (gewoonliknie meer as 6 nie). verwysings verwysings in die teks moet voorsien word van die skrywer se van en die datum, b.v., van riper (1971). wanneer daar egter meer as twee skrywers is, moet daar na die eerste verskaffing van al die outeurs, van "et al." gebruik gemaak word. in die geval waar daar egter ses of meer outeurs ter sprake moet "et al." van die begin af gebruik word. al die name van die skrywers moet in die verwysingslys verskyn wat aan die einde van die artikel voorkom. verwysings moet alfabeties in trippel spasiering gerangskik word. al die verwysings moet in die verwysingslys verskyn, insluitende sekondire bronne, ("αρα pub. man." 1983, p. 13). slegs aanvaarbare afkortings van tydskrifte se titels mag gebruik word, (sien "dsh abstracts, october"; of "the world list of scientific periodicals"). die aantal verwyings moet nie meer as 25 oorskrei nie, tensy dit geregverdig is. let op die volgende voorbeelde: locke, j.l. (1983). clinical psychology: the explanation and treatment of speech sound disorders./. speech hear. disord., 48, 339-341. penrod, j.p. (1985). speech discrimination testing. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiologg (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. davis, g.a. & wilcox, m.j. (1985). adult aphasia rehabilitation: applied pragmatics. san diego, ca.: college-hill. redigering manuskripte wat aanvaar is, mag na die skrywer terruggestuur word vir hersiening. addisionele kleiner veranderinge mag ook op hierdie stadium aangebring word, maar 'n nota ter aanduiding van alle veranderinge wat op die manuskrip voorkom, moet verskafword. die artikel word dan aan die redaksionele komitee vir finale redigering van styl, duidelikheid en konsekwentheid teruggestuur. herdrukke: 10 herdrukke sonder omslae sal gratis aan die outeurs verskaf word. sluitingsdatum vir bydraes: bydraes word verkieslik teen 31 mei elke jaar verwag, maar artikels sal nog tot 31 julie vir aanvaarding oorweeg word. navrae, korrespondensie en manuskripte: moet geadresseer word aan die redakteur, die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, die suid-afrikaanse spraak-taal-gehoor vereeniging, posbus 31782, braamfontein, 2017, suid afrika. p r i n t e d b y v 4 r p r i n t i n g w o r k s , p r e t o r i a r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a study of european speech defective school children in pretoria l. weiss, b.a. log. m.a. the scope of this study: 63 schools with a total school population of 33,482 children were studied. these excluded all special schools and all schools not administered by the transvaal education department. among these children, 1,513 were found to have speech defects severe enough to enable teachers to diagnose the children as having "something" wrong with their speech. this figure represents 4.5 per cent, of the school population studied. findings 1. among the speech defective children studied 38 per cent, were dyslalic, and 28 per cent, were dysphemic. 2. the ratio among the sexes was on an average 2.2 boys to 1 girl increasing to 3 boys to one girl among the dysphemics. 3. some interesting differences were found in the incidence of speech defectives children in different types of schools. it was found that the largest incidence occurred in the afrikaans medium high schools, 5.2 per cent, while the lowest incidence of only 2.2 per cent, occurred in the english medium high schools. the results though very interesting and giving us much food for thought, are not completely reliable, since surveys were in most instances done by school teachers, and since there are very few english medium high schools in pretoria. the discrepancy in the primary schools was not as great, the afrikaans school with 4.8 per cent, and the english schools again lower with 3.7 per cent. dual medium schools cannot be compared as only three schools were available for study, although it can be stated that the incidence of spaech defectives was higher than among any of the other types of schools. 4. it was also found that youngest children in the family had a significantly higher incidence of speech defects, than children in other positions. results of therapy: this included only a few schools, where children were treated once or twice a week, for a period from 6 to 10 months. response to therapy was on the whole very gratifying, 92 per cent, of the children showing definite improvement, including almost 50 per cent, who were completely rehabilitated in such a short time. may i conclude by stating my firm belief that the speech defective school children must be helped, and that we can help him to attain his rightful place in the community by helping him while he is still at school. ride & a i e t g f) the greatest name in cycling o n terms 7s. 6d. weekly from all raleigh cycle dealers r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 journal of the south african logopedic society december organic d y s l a l i a by alice rummel, b.a. log rand speech is the highest and most fundamental human attribute and without it one is set outside the possibilities of constant communication with his fellows. speech is also an important tool for social adjustment, and must conform to the environment. when listeners recognise speech as defective, impaired social relationships arise which further inhibit and distort speech. as the defect gets progressively worse the individual's social relationships become impaired. thus it is not sufficient to know only what kind of defect the person has, but also what kind of person has a speech defect. the speech defect alone has no meaning but must be considered in relationship to the whole personality, since speech and personality are closely linked. there are four aspects necessary for the production of normal speech: 1. phonatory — concerned with the voice or laryngeally controlled air stream. 2. articulatory — here the stream is modified by articulatory organs of the mouth and throat. 3. linguistic — which must conform to an arbitrary code of meanings for communication. 4. auditory. in these processes of vocal control and articulatory modification, the chief guide "as to the accuracy of conformity to the code is the oral communication of others, this interpretation being an integral part of the function of speech." many faculties are concerned in the completion of this fourfold picture. if one fails to develop, speech fails, even though the other functions are normal. psychologically, articulation, which is the moulding of speech sounds, is not a special faculty in itself, but is rather an amalgamation of several abilities that develop side by side during the first years of life. it is largely these abilities that determine the onset of speech. if one of these abilities are late in dteveloping, speech will be delayed, defective or both. the factors underlying articulation are both sensory and motor. they are:— 1. discrimination and acuity to sounds of high frequency. 2. memory span for individual speech sounds. 3. speed of muscle movement. 4. specialization of movement. froeschels and jellinek define dyslalia as concerning "all disorders occurring in articulation. if sounds are produced1 incorrectly or replaced by others, or if they are entirely lacking, then the person is dyslalic". the classification of dyslalia is mainly based on etiology. there are three main divisions: a. organic. b. functional. c. psychological. these divisions, however, may overlap and are interrelated, and have no clearly defined boundaries. there is seldom an organic factor alone, but usually there is some introduction of a psychological factor. organic dyslalia presupposes structural deformity or abnormality and thus implies a disorder of articulation due directly to the fact that some part or parts of the articulatory apparatus are so defective anatomically and physiologically that they are unable to perform their primary functions of chewing, sucking and swallowing, or their secondary derived function of articulating with a great degree of perfection. there are any number of possible oral deformities, e.g. cleft lip, paralysis. of the lip or lip inadequacy due to shortness, trauma or other cause; abnormalities in size, symmetry or functional efficiency of the tongue, tongue-tie, growttis on the tongue and paralysis; disturbed dental relationships such as all types of malocclusion, spaced or missing, misplaced or supernumerary teeth or other dental conditions that may interfere with usual articulatory adjustments necessary for good speech production; cleft palate, abnormally highly arched palate, and short cleft or paralysed velum. these are some possible causes of organic dyslalia. a deformity of any one of the structures of the oral region tends to cause a distortion of the other adjacent structures, since their anatomical and functional relationships are so close. because of these close relationships, the condition and functioning of the oral structures should be carefully noted, in order to determine their possible role in causing the given speech defect. if the basis of the speech defect is organic, surgical and medical attention may be needed; if it is functional or psychological in origin, the treatment should proceed from the approach indicated. examination procedures van riper states that the mouth and throat of all cases should be examined. although the case may effect a good acoustic result of speech in spite of a physical anomaly, such an organic abnormality will be some handicap in speech r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society development. the presence of an organic abnormality is not important unless it stands in functional relationship to defective speech sounds. diagnosis should not be made hastily. other causal factors may also be of great importance besides the organic defect. each part of the articulatory organs should be examined thoroughly. the structure itself should be examined not only in quiescence but also in its relation to the appropriate speech sounds. all evidence of a handicapping abnormality and all compensatory movements should be recorded. other factors such as intelligence, auditory acuity, motor inco-ordinations and especially emotional hi^ory and background of the case should be taken into consideration. defects caused by mouth deformities are usually structural in origin. however, the deformity of the mouth is not always the explanation for the speech defect. thus: 1. structural anomalies of organs of articulation may occur but there is no speech defect. 2. acceptable speech can be produced by adjustments of the articulatory organs which are mechanically quite different from adjustments usually made in speech. 3. there is more than one way of producing a particular speech sound. for example, in the production of the " t " sound, if the patient cannot elevate the tip of the tongue he can use the blade. the severity of the speech involvement depends upon the extent of the abnormality, and also upon the degree of compensation by the articulatory organs before the restoration of the function of the imperfect structures has been achieved. the ability to compensate differs with every individual case even if the extent of the deformity is similar. this difference may depend on differences in auditory awareness of speech peculiarity, kinesthetic sensitivity, intelligence, motivation and environmental stimulation. van riper suggests that bright children from homes with high speech standards will learn compensatory methods more easily than those less inligent children from homes with poor speech standards. this ability to compensate involves the child's residual diathesis or x-factor. the commonly-accepted postulate is that the usual adjustment of the articulators used for a given sound is the easiest, but by no means the only possible adjustment by which the given sound may be produced. although a person has a structural abnormality and cannot employ the usual manner of articulation, he may learn a new method for producing the sound, thus developing a unique skill. thus his structural abnormality does not interfere with his speech. but if the individual lacks this ability to acquire unique skills, his speech will be defective. in his speech difficulty then due to his structural anomaly or to his lack of facility? this facility is complex — "made up of mutually supporting factors such as the individual's health, endocrine drive, general vitality, intelligence, hearing acuity, efficiency of motor co-ordination, motivation and training for good speech afforded by his social environment." if these factors are in his favour, he can make successful compensations for any marked structural deviations. if these factors are against him a slight deviation may prove more of an obstacle than he can overcome without special help. this is not the only important factor in the individual with congenital malformations and other structural abnormalities. these anomalies themselves may be a specially, narrowly localized deviation and present an uncomplicated problem, or the anomaly may be evidence of a constitutional inferiority, with characteristic reduction of impetus in any project which requires individual initiative and stamina. thus in drawing up a programme for speech rehabilitation, this xfactor must be taken into consideration. it should be considered in the broad spheres when evaluating the child, not only in his ability to learn speech, but also his reactions in meeting other demands for initiative, resourcefulness and stamina. van riper says that there are two methods for minimizing the influence of organic abnormalities: 1. reconstruction of defective organic structure through surgery and orthodontia. 2. teaching of compensatory movements in speech sound production. orthodontia has seen great advancement and changes in dental, palatal and jaw structures have now been accomplished. cases, especially children with marked mouth deformities should be referred to the appropriate specialists so that reconstruction work can be carried out. thus effort will not be wasted in teaching compensatory movements where medical or dental care should first be instituted. palatal abnormalities are frequently associated with those of the jaws and orthodontic projection or retraction of the jaw can facilitate tongue contact with the roof of the mouth. a wide variety of surgical repair and construction work can be carried out, e.g. scar tissue can be excised and grafts can be made which will provide the necessary mobility. high palatal arches can be lowered and the velum can be modified. the most desirable time for surgery is during childhood. the speech therapist should recommend to the parents that it be done and see that it is carried out. the paralyzed structures can occasionally be helped by exercise adminisr ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 journal of the south african logopedic society december tered by a physiotherapist. there is usually recourse to more biological functions and tying up of the specialized movement with gross muscular action. the case may also need a hearing aid. the teaching of compensatory movements can be initiated if the case cannot be helped by surgery or orthodontia. since all speech sounds may be produced in a variety of ways, e.g. the art of the ventriloquist demonstrates compensatory activity of that of the lips and jaws. normal speakers may have profound anatomical abnormalities and produce sounds with a perfect acoustic end result. one must take into account and try to minimize the following factors in teaching compensatory movements: (i) complexity of performance (the fewer adjustments the better); (ii) ease of transition from other sounds; (iii) amount of facial distortion; (iv) distinctness of kinesthetic and tactual sensations ; (v) motivation and co-operation of the subject; an organic anomaly often causes unfavourable emotional and social attitudes. the reason for this is that any peculiarity of appearance, behaviour or speech tends to interfere with favourable adjustment. these attitudes may be prevented or minimized by adequate structural repair before the child can become sensitive about his appearance. reactions to deformities should be made objective. let the child face his problem squarely and learn to deal with any difficulties which he may encounter. on the other hand, the child's abnormality must not be kept in consciousness all the time. parents and teachers should stress other social graces, e.g. good grooming, friendly mannerisms, conversational ability, music, etc. therapy: all possible medical, surgical and orthopraxic procedures must be tried as early in childhood as possible because: 1. it prevents fixation of defective motor patterns in the nervous system, which will be displaced only with great difficulty, if at all. 2. it allows deformed parts an opportunity of reshaping into a normal mould after surgical repair by means of the natural processes of growth. 3it brings cosmetic improvement very early and therefore avoids the social insecurity and selfconsciousness brought about by physical deformities. if normal structure has not been obtained through medical, surgical or dental care, training should begin as early as possible to teach the child functional compensations for his structural deficiencies, and differences from the normal. "mechanically, standard speech is dependent upon normal anatomical structure." when anatomy is not normal, functional compensations should imitate as nearly as possible the mechanics of the correct sound rather than its placement in the mouth. after successful surgical repair, previous compensations must be eliminated, unless they are mechanically efficient in the production of the desired sounds. an isolated sound may be formed in a non-standard way with some facility, but when combined with other sounds or into blends these non-standard methods may not produce the desired acoustic result. thus the compensatory method must be considered as to wether it can be used together with other sounds, i.e. its usefulness for ordinary conversational speech. if structural anomalies of teeth, tongue and mouth remain, after surgery, compensations must be developed. the decision should only be made after careful analysis based on knowledge of mechanical properties. however, one must keep in mind the individuality of each case. thus in a programme of treatment and training the surgical repair comes first. the case can then proceed to exercises to strengthen the parts required, and go on to the correction of defective sounds. the prognosis depends on the severity of the deformity and on the efficiency of repair and amount of speech training. the operation alone may have no noticeable effect on his speech. postoperative training is a great deal more than half of his speech rehabilitation. complete or even partial surgical repair may be impossible. also it may be impossible to wholly compensate for any deficiencies by mechanical means. this needs special speech training. often the very badly deformed cases require not so much direct speech training as mental hygiene. conclusion. in this article an attempt has been made to cover some aspects in the field of organic dyslalia. details of the many defects of organic dyslalia have been avoided. instead a short resume indicating some important aspects of the field have been given. the importance of organic factors is obvious, and should not be overlooked, nor must too much emphasis be laid on them, because an individual may display normal speech in spite of organic abnormality. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech therapy for the cerebral palsied child a plan of treatment based on the bobath technique by g l e n d a s h a p i r o b.a. log. (rand) and r u t h j a c o b s b.a. log. (rand) (with acknowledgement to the principal and management committee of the forest town school for cerebral palsied children) the speech therapist engaged in treating the cerebral palsied child has usually looked to the sphere of physiotherapy for guidance and help, and has always reaped the benefit of the techniques and principles propounded by such eminent authorities as phelps, collis and temple fay. now, once again, the speech therapist can benefit from a physiotherapy technique, and gain a wealth of knowledge to help her in her therapy with the cerebral palsied child. here the writers refer to the approach to treatment devised by mrs. b. bobath. t h e t h e r a p e u t i c p r o g r a m m e general principles 1. the therapist must endeavour to normalise the muscle tone of the child's whole body in the reflex inhibiting posture (r.i.p.) before she attempts to teach speech to the child. the speech therapist should employ all the r.i.p.'s that will normalise muscle tone in the spine, neck, head, tongue, peripheral speech mechanism and prevertebral muscles. cerebral palsy is not a difficulty concerning individual muscles, but is rather a deficiency in movement and co-ordination, as the nervous impulses to the muscles are not directed in the normal way. there is also lack of inhibition from the higher centres in the central nervous system. if the therapist concentrates on treating only one muscle, she will probably produce spasm in other muscles, thus often stimulating spasticity in other parts of the body. the athetoid child must be taught to maintain muscle tone and control and grade movement. 2. training through the visual sensory field is used more as an aid than as a basic technique. preferably, training should be done through the proprioceptive pathways. cerebral palsy is a sensory motor handicap, and the cerebral palsied child usually has a deficient or distorted sensory intake. therefore the therapist should encourage the teaching of the "sensations" of normal movement and use the visual pathways as an aid only. 3. the child unhampered by the difficulties produced by cerebral palsy usually achieves head control at about six months, and at this time begins to babble. one cannot have a selective activity without first achieving fixation. in the cerebral palsied child the head and neck must be controlled before speech can function as an adequate means of communication. thus, one may summarise the problem, and say that the foundation stone of speech therapy for the cerebral palsied child is to first achieve head and neck control. the therapist must at first try to achieve total inhibition of any movement of the body which effects the head or neck. at a later stage the child should be able to speak and move simultaneously without spasm. ultimately the aim should be to create a speech function which is independent of the rest of the body. 4. the speech therapist must not allow any deterioration of muscle tone while giving her speech lessons. it is of the utmost importance that the speech therapist is aware of the spasm that can and does occur when the child initiates speech. this spasm must be inhibited. therefore, the therapist must first inhibit the primitive reflex patterns, and aim to build up higher and more organised reflex and voluntary activity on a developmental scale. 5. breathing should be taught on the basis of releasing spasm or normalising tone. bobath suggests that depending on where the spasm occurs, the therapist should manipulate and vibrate either the spine, thorax or larynx to improve exhalation. the concept of blowing exercises to improve breathing has been found to create diaphragmatic, thoracic and head/neck spasms. this, of course, is most undesirable, and can only cause further handicap to the child. instead the vibratory movement initiated by the therapist, as in the bobath method, helps to achieve relaxed breathing and frequently a long sustained sound without pressure or blocking occurs. ! 6. once' vocalisation has been induced and if the child is in a well controlled r.i.p. and vocalises spontaneously, the therapist must facilitate speech journal of the african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sounds by manipulating the tongue, lips and face. the therapist must be careful not to reinforce wrong patterns of posture or movement. if the child has an extensor spasm he must be positioned so that the head is in the midline or flexed. similarly if the pattern is one of flexion, the head must be up-right. usually the child with an extensor spasm will articulate "g" and "k" sounds more easily than he will other sounds. the child with flexor spasticity will find the "b", "p" and " m " sounds easier to articulate. the easiest sound to facilitate is "b". the voiceless sounds are the most difficult ones to initiate and should be left to the last stages of therapy. vowels should be taught first and then consonants, and finally the combinations of vowels and consonants. alveolar sounds are facilitated, by the therapist moving her finger up and down under the front of the mandible. this forces the tongue up to the alveolar ridge. velar sounds are obtained by moving the finger up and down under the mandible towards the back of the tongue. the other sounds are facilitated in a similar way depending on where they are made in the mouth. 7. the therapist must allow the child to experience all the stages of normal speech development. thus, therapy may follow the same phases of development as in the normal child, based on the preliminary feeding patterns of chewing, sucking and swallowing. the child is placed in the r.i.p. most suitable for him and is helped by the therapist to chew, suck and swallow. hard foods are used for chewing exercises, while tubing and straws are used to help the child to suck. the swallowing reflex is stimulated by the therapist massaging and gently pressing the front of the throat, whilst the lips are held together. at all times the therapist must observe where the spasm occurs, and then position the child accordingly. the technique of "upside down feeding" is also used if the child drools incessantly. another way of obtaining lip closure to reduce drooling, and when the muscle tone is low, is by the technique of "pinching" round the mouth. if the child continually bites when stimulated on the front and sides of the gums, this primitive "bite" reflex must be inhibited by the therapist placing the child in a suitable r.i.p., holding the jaw down and stimulating the area round the lips, teeth and gums. when these areas have eventually been desensitised so that the child will not automatically bite when stimulated, the grip on the chin area is gradually reduced and finally eliminated. 8. the abnormal movements of the tongue must be corrected. it is of no avail to drill the cerebral palsied child with endless tongue exercises, without first reducing the spasm or overflow movements in the tongue, i.e. the tongue must be relaxed and controlled through an r.i.p. which normalises muscle tone in the head, neck and spinal regions. it has been found that it is far easier to achieve control of the tongue by first establishing jaw control. one of the key points, in this connection, is under the chin. here, once again muscle tone must be normalised. if the child has a forward tongue thrust the therapist must position the child so that his chin is not in a downward position, resting on the chest. conversely if there is an extensor thrust of the head and neck, paper may be placed under the chin. the child must hold this in position while the therapist endeavours to pull it away. this is for learning control of the extensor spasm in the neck. 9. voice quality can be affected by any change of muscle tone. spasms occurring in the laryngeal areas cause considerable deviations in the quality of the voice. since the athetoid and ataxic child have fluctuating muscle tone, one may find the voice quality and volume to be equally fluctuating. the vocal quality of the spastic child is often whispered and jerky. when treating the spastic child the stimulus must be graded e.g. beginning with a slow movement and increasing the speed, as greater speed will give rise to increased tone. with the athetoid and hyperkenetic child the therapist positions the child, gradually reducing her hold and eventually gives control over to the child. control is always from proximal to distal with the spine being stimulated for head control. 10. the speech therapist should use more than one reflex inhibiting posture during a therapy session. these r.i.p.'s follow the movement patterns of the development of the normal child. what is achieved in one position, should also be tried in another position. there must be a carry-over into all positions such as sitting, standing etc. to achieve such a carry-over, the therapist should first initiate various movements of the limbs, and the shoulder/neck regions whilst the child is vocalising. conclusion the writers wish to point out that the above discussion represents only one of the methods used in their treatment of the cerebral palsied speech defective child. other techniques are employed concomitantly with the bobath technique. june, 1961 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) march j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society 3 some general aspects concerning stuttering which indicate fields of research by m. aron m.a. t o consider in any detail the information that is contained in the literature on stuttering is a formidable and confusing task. it includes broadly the many and conflicting theories as to its etiology, the nature and development of the symptom itself (often theoretical), the stutterer himself in relation to his handicap and the attitudes of the audience to the stutterer and his symptom. much time has been spent on the constitutional (physio-neurological) concomitants that attempt to explain the phenomenon. the psychological element has also been considered in some detail. however, the writer has the impression that there is comparatively little information on stuttering that could be viewed from a cultural and sociological standpoint. the limited material that is available only seems to emphasize the neglect of this field. the purpose of this paper is to briefly discuss, by way of the literature, some factors related to stuttering that indicate further fields of research — particularly those fields that have cultural and psycho-sociological implications. i n c i d e n c e the greater majority of studies conducted to ascertain the incidence of stuttering have, unfortunately, been limited to the united states. there are relatively few studies from other parts of the world. the average incidence taken from 15 sources (1) is 0.80% — the highest being 3.5% and the lowest 0.55%. apart from the united states and england, the writer has only discovered studies from cyprus (1.85%), denmark (0.90%), accra, gold coast (3.5%) and westphalia, germany (1.00%). t h e average figure of 0.80% is probably not accurate as the data from the studies have been derived from varied approaches. the methods used, the criteria of selection, the age-groups studied etc., appear to differ from study to study. the incidence figure itself does not appear to be very important to our understanding of the stuttering problem. what does appear highly relevant is whether stuttering is a universal disorder, and whether the incidence varies markedly in any way in various parts of the world. attention was first drawn to this particular aspect when snidecor (2) in 1947 reported that no stuttering existed among the bannock and shoshone indians of north america, and neither did they have a word to describe stuttering. on the other hand, lemert (3) in 1953, reported that stuttering did exist among three indian groups of the northwest pacific coast. w o r d s describing stuttering were found in the languages of these people. this discrepancy between groups of north american indians is perhaps not so surprising when one considers the cultural differences between them. sheeh an (4) felt that there was more similarity between the culture of the kwakiutl indians (among whom stuttering was found by lemert) and our own culture than there is between the cultures of the bannock and shoshone indians and the kwakiutls. he stated that the latter tribe has a competitive culture not unlike our own. morgenstern (5) has given us some information about the non-existence of stuttering among some groups. he received his information from anthropologists and other workers in various parts of the world. there appear to be six main groups that do not exhibit stuttering and neither do any of these groups have a word in their languages to describe the stuttering phenomenon. the groups are: 1) the napishianas of british guiana. 2) the patamanas and akawaio of british guiana. 3) the garia in the territory of new guinea. 4) the kelabits. west borneo. 5) the malayan aborigines, malaya. 6) the sonthals, bhuyana and gatwas, (from behar) and the turis and tantis (from orissa) assam, india. however, it is felt that the reports of these workers (not speech pathologists) must be taken tentatively until objective studies can be done to either corroborate or refute this information. should it be correct, then it seems appropriate that detailed cultural and psychor ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y sociological studies of these groups should be done. the results of such studies might very well throw light on factors associated with stuttering in other culture groups. johnson (6) considers that the non-existence of a word to describe stuttering among the bannock and shoshone indians suggests a significant explanation for the absence of stuttering among these people. his semantogenic diagnosogenic theories as to the onset of stuttering account for this. the writer feels that the non-existence of a word to describe stuttering is not necessarily an etiological factor. it is feasible to argue that a disorder like stuttering must first be detected by a group of people and then a word created to describe the observation. however, it is agreed that the use of the term "stuttering" in the evaluations that people make concerning their childrens' speech (including the normal non-fluencies that are exhibited by children) can contribute greatly to the onset of stuttering. remarking on the fact that some cultures do not exhibit stuttering as we know it, lemert (7) said this measurably strengthens the hypothesis that cultural settings are dynamic factors in the growth of stuttering. . . . the process by which cultural values are sieved through family organization before they impinge upon the child is an important variable in any explanation of stuttering. it would be helpful to know how other culture groups regard the development of speech in their children and the role that language plays in their various societies. this area is plainly a fertile ground for future research, t h e s e x r a t i o the particular kind of sex ratio that occcurs among stutterers continues to be a puzzle. writers have firmly established that stuttering occurs more frequently among males than females. some studies have reported a ratio of approximately 3:1 and others approximately 5:1 (1). apart from the organic constitutional factors that have been put forward to explain the differences between the sexes (earlier myelinization in girls, motoric differences in favour of girls, etc), cultural factors appear to play an important role. the most important premise to consider is that females tend to develop language at an earlier rate than males and that the co-ordination necessary for the speech act appears to be established earlier in girls. this has been explained in terms of constitutional differences. however, some have put forward the hypothesis that this difference might be due to the different parental attitudes towards the sexes. these attitudes are a reflection of the society we live in. our culture tends to place more responsibility on the male child in order to prepare him for manhood and the role of the bread-winner. it is conceivable that, in certain cases, the pressures that are brought to bear by our society (mirrored in the parents) become too great for the young male child who might still be constitutionally immature. the standards of speech and speech fluency might prove too high for certain children and this could possibly cause a breakdown in the very area in which the child is inadequate, namely the motor act of speech. the sex ratio of stutterers has been dealt with in some detail by schuell (8). the data that she found in one of her studies led her to believe that the attitudes of parents are culturally determined, rather than being based upon actual observation of behaviour of a particular child. she has said the following: it seems clear that insecurities, anxieties and tensions would tend to be increased by the contradictory attitudes prevalent in our culture; first, the demand that the male child exhibit independence, fearlessness, selfcontrol and a certain amount of aggressiveness; and second, that he must at. the same time be submissive, orderly, and obedient; that he must conform to standards of parents and teachers derived from attitudes diametrically opposed to those which clinical psychologists consider conducive to healthful modes of behaviour. schuell said further that she felt that the crucial point seemed to lie in the tendency of parents and teachers to expect equal performance from children who are not developmentally equal. her conclusion about the sex differences, she feels, suggests johnson's theory that stuttering is a semantogenic and diagnosogenic disorder. the child, to some extent, interiorizes the negative evaluations made by his immediate adult society. morgenstern (5) reports that some of the data sent to him from various parts of the world indicated that the sex ratio is of the same type as that for stutterers in other communities. he found it difficult to believe that the same purely environmental (culturally determined) stimuli which might create a higher incidence of stuttering among boys than girls in our society, would be found in other culture groups, some of which differ considerably in terms of social customs and mores. t h e writer believes that the sex ratio is organically rather than culturally determined. cultural or environmental influences may "trigger-off" or aggrevate inadequate physical r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) march j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society elements to which the male child appears to be more prone. it would be interesting to speculate whether the incidence of stuttering will be any higher for females in our civilization after a number of generations. females are assuming fuller responsibility and independence in our society and it is possible that this may cause changes in parental attitudes towards the young female child. however, more detailed study of our culture and others in this regard may throw more light on the general problem of the sex ratio. little has been written about the effects of our culture on the onset or originality of stuttering. still less is known about other culture groups that do or do not stutter. some cognizance should be taken of those society groups that do not stutter. the attitudes expressed toward the sexes, the language standards, etc., in these groups may become relevant points of departure for further analysis of the sex ratio for stutterers in those communities that do stutter. s c h o l a s t i c r e t a r d a t i o n it has been acknowledged by writers that stutterers experience the school situation as being difficult and threatening. some investigations have indicated that stutterers are scholastically retarded. studies have reported retardation ranging from 6 months up to 1.6 years (1). sources have indicated that the level of intelligence has not been found to be a causative factor for this retardation (1). the main reason that has been put forward to explain the scholastic retardation is that the stuttering child is likely to lack a measure of concentration due to his being prone to be excitable, restless and threatened by the demands made in the classroom. demands for oral work are frequent, and the stutterer is placed in an emotionally charged situation where he is expected to answer questions or recite before his class mates. the writer has observed that stutterers are frequently marked down because of their inability to express themselves or to recite adequately. it has also been observed that some parents of stutterers are inclined to enrol their child in school at a. slightly later age. it is presumed that this is done to allow for a period of time in which the stutterer may "outgrow" his speech difficulty. the reasons that have been suggested to explain the scholastic retardation of stutterers . tend to be psychologically orientated. children jf^ who are inclined to be withdrawn, or non|i'|j. fluent (but not stuttering), or excitable when | j | | called upon to express themselves, may c o n j a i l ceivably react adversely to the abuse ana re-jh® marks that others in the classroom and p l a y s s | | ground express. the abuse offered by children.imsa it is felt, follows closely the nature and evaluations associated with the social customs of the particular society they live in. the information available on this aspect still needs to be verified by further study. insufficient data has been accumulated to make this a definite factor associated with stuttering. late enrolment in schools and the problem of intelligence should both be investigated in fuller detail .workers would also need to know the frequency of poor marks being given to the stutterer due to his impediment, the extent to which these poor marks contribute towards failure, the attitudes of the .staff to this particular speech problem and the methods used to handle them in the class-room situation. this type of information might guide the speech therapist and lend weight to recommendations which can be made to the staff of schools a s to the various methods of handling the stutterer. s o c i o e c o n o m i c f a c t o r s some mention has been made in the literature of the effects of socio-economic factors influencing the incidence of stuttering. it is thought that the particular level of socioeconomic standing of a family group would affect the type of attitudes displayed towards the children and thereby affect the standards of speech in the home. it has been observed that children from the upper social levels show a greater linguistic development, use larger sentences and have more mature sentence forms at earlier ages than the children in homes of low socioeconomic standing. (9) the type of environment and attitudes displayed by parents which afford few stimulating opportunities for effective speech learning can occur, of course, at all socio-economic levels, but are more inclined to do so in the poorer and more deprived homes. it is noted by darely (10) that parents of stutterers came predominantly from the middle and upper socio-economic classes. these parents tended to be better educated than their control 'group counterparts and they " . . . tended to set rather high standards for themselves, their mates, their children and their neighbours." he also reported that the parents of stutterers expressed more dissatisfaction with their own abilities and accomplishments than did the control group of parents. johnson (11) commented on darely's observations and said that what was probably more important than the socio-economic class was \" . . . the degree of 'upward mobility' (drive or intensity of determination and effort to rise socio-economically) of the family." families that are "upwardly mobile" may, suggested [johnson, be expected to experience to a greater r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society than usual degree the tensions attendant upon competitiveness to place high values on the absence of defects and good or superior speech in their children, which, " . . . they consider important in the competition for status." morgenstern (5) found similar data as reported above. a higher incidence of stuttering was noted among children of semi-skilled manual workers than among children of unskilled labourers in england. semi-skilled workers appeared to have a better chance of 'getting ahead" and were more socially orientated as regards status level. morgenstern felt, therefore, that the semi-skilled labourer was likely to be ambitious for his children and to prize the advantage of adequate speech. evaluations made of a child's speech and the degree to which he is pushed to develop " g o o d " speech, which may be beyond his capacity, may cause the child to react adversely to this strain and therefore may result in tension and stuttering. on the other hand some writers (12, 13) have found no evidence that stuttering is influenced by social status or extremes of environmental conditions. t h e knowledge available concerning the influence of socio-economic levels on the incidence of stuttering is limited and comparatively recent. it appears that the main influence of a high socio-economic level in a home is that high standards will be set for children in regard to the expectations that are made for conforming behaviour and for adequate or "superior" speech. these high expectations can create tensions in the child, resulting in anxiety and a possible awareness of the speech activity that he performs. the consideration of this aspect would indicate that cultural and psychological components are operating as a possible precipitating factor. difficulties, it is felt, will be encountered in assessing this aspect as this type of investigation involves the study of various social strata which are dynamic in nature and continually changing. s t u t t e r i n g a m o n g t h e b a n t u the above mentioned factors, among others that have been associated with stuttering and that can be considered from a cultural and sociological point of view, were taken by the writer as a basis for an investigation on the nature and incidence of stuttering among a bantu group of school-going children. a set field programme was organised and each school in selected urban african residential areas was .carefully screened for stutterers. each stutterer was then interviewed in some detail, observed for symptoms, and where possible, home visits were made. the data collected and evaluated indicated that the incidence of stuttering appears to be the same for any group of stutterers that we know of, namely 1.28%. the sex ratio, onset of stuttering, familial incidence of stuttering, the symptoms exhibited, the methods by which the subjects attempted to eliminate the disorder, are all similiar to what other investigators have reported. n o real differences were found to exist that would differentiate this group of stutterers from any other studied. there are many words, some onomatopoeic in nature, in all the languages that describe the phenomenon of stuttering. the invesigation, however, had many limitations. due to it being an exploratory study, no experimental groups (controls) were used. the information offered as to the background of the subjects was often inconsistent and could not be checked. parents were not always available for interviewing. the use of an african social worker trained to participate as an interpreter did not eradicate the social and language barriers that existed. it is felt that further study of this problem must now follow. there is a vast field of research for speech therapists in this country because of the almost unique situation that exists among the bantu. for there are large communities of bantu still leading comparatively simple lives. on the other hand, there are groups who have been urbanized for several generations and have, to varying degrees, assimilated the demands of urban life. further, a most fruitful situation to consider, is the almost perceptible changes that are taking place due to the transition of the rural african to urban life. these three broad types of situations could provide excellent material for the purposes of comparison — particularly in regard to the influences of these various environments on such aspects as are related to our work. the speech standards set in the homes, the influence of the many languages on individuals, the customs revolving around the general subject of language and speech all become important factors to observe in these comparative situations. w e can only surmise whether the incidence of stuttering among the rural and urban bantu would be the same. provision for control groups should be made, especially control groups in the various environmental strata — rural, urban and transitional. due to the difficulties encountered it would seem necessary and relevant for a team of workers to approach this field of research. such a team, it is envisaged, should include not only speech pathologists, but psychologists and sociologists among others. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) march j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society c o n c l u s i o n s the above account clearly indicates that further study is necessary to fully understand the factors that have been associated with stuttering. comparatively few workers have investigated the disorder in sociological terms and this field appears to be neglected as an area of research. in addition, far too few studies have dealt with groups of people in parts of the world other than america, england and a few european countries. this pertains particularly to the groups of primitive peoples that still exist. most of the material available on stuttering seems to have originated at the desks of writers and in the laboratories attached to clinics. it might be argued that the psychological studies dealing with stuttering consider the sociological point of view. however, it is felt that the term "psychological" is becoming too broad in nature and does not take fully into account the dynamic cultural and social reactions, positive and negative, among people in any group. it is not claimed that further data from a sociological and cultural point of view would solve the riddle of stuttering. but it is thought that we can become more aware of the influences of the social environment which could direct us in making further more positive attempts in dealing with the prophylaxis of this disorder. it would seem a tragedy to ignore the many fields still open for study. the world is rapidly changing and primitive peoples are gradually becoming assimilated to other ways of life. malinowski (14) one of the great anthropologists of our time, has said that within a generation or two the native communities now available for scientific study will have practically disappeared — " t h e need for energetic work is urgent, and the time is short." 6. j o h n s o n , wendell. " p e o p l e in q u a n d a r i e s . " h a r p e r a n d b r o s . , n e w york, 1946. 7. l e m e r t , ε. m. " s o c i a l p a t h o l o g y , " c h a p t e r 6. mcgraw-hill, n e w york, 1951. 8. schuell, η. m. " s e x differences in r e l a t i o n to s t u t t e r i n g : p a r t i . " j . s p e e c h d i s o r d e r s . vol. ii, n o . 4, 1946. 9. mccarthy, dorothea. " l a n g u a g e development in c h i l d r e n . " chapter 9 in m a n u a l ol child p s y c h o l o g y , ed. carraichael, l . second e d i t i o n . j o h n wiley & sons, inc. n e w york. 1954. 10. darely, f . l . " t h e r e l a t i o n s h i p of p a r e n t a l a t t i t u d e s a n d a d j u s t m e n t s to the development of s t u t t e r i n g . " chapter 4. s t u t t e r i n g in children and a d u l t s ed. j o h n son wendell. u n i v e r s i t y of m i n n e s o t a p r e s s , minneapolis, 1955. 11. j o h n s o n , wendell. " s p e e c h h a n d i c a p p e d school childr e n . " r e v i s e d e d i t i o n . h a r p e r b r o s . , n e w york. 1956. 12. morley, muriel. " t h e d e v e l o p m e n t a n d d i s o r d e r s of speech in c h i l d h o o d . " e . & s. l i v i n g s t o n e l t d . , e d i n b u r g h . 1957. 13. mcallister, a. h. " c l i n i c a l s t u d i e s in speech t h e r a p y . " u n i v e r s i t y of l o n d o n p r e s s , london. 1937. 14. malinowski, b r o n i s l a w . " a r g o n a u t s of the w e s t e r n p a c i f i c . " george r o u t l e d g e a n d sons, l t d . , l o n d o n . 1922. westdene products ( p t y . ) l t d . specialists in medical literature recommend— speech problems of children edited by wendell johnson, price 38/3 this book offers authorative information on speech disorders; it points the way to a full, normal and well-adjusted life for uncounted thousands of children. the chapters express the viewpoints and reflect the practical experience of the authors and of 20 other experts. order your copy now! johannesburg: 23 essanby house, 175 jeppe street. cape town: 408 grand parade centre, castle street. durban: 60/67 national mutual building smith street. pretoria: 210 medical centre, pretorlus st. b i b l i o g r a p h y 1. aron, m. l . " a n i n v e s t i g a t i o n of the n a t u r e and incidence of s t u t t e r i n g a m o n g a b a n t u group of s c h o o l g o i n g c h i l d r e n . " m.a. d i s s e r t a t i o n . u n i v e r s i t y of the w i t w a t e r s r a n d . 1958. 2. snidecor, j . c. " w h y the i n d i a n d o e s not s t u t t e r . " quart. j . s p e e c h , vol. 33, 1947. 3. l e m e r t , ε . m. " s o m e i n d i a n s w h o s t u t t e r . " j . s p e e c h h e a r i n g d i s o r d e r s . vol. 18. n o . 2, 1953. 4. sheehan, j . " f i l m a n d d i s c u s s i o n on s t u t t e r i n g . " copy of t a l k given to the s t a f f of the l o s a n g e l e s mental h y g i e n e clinic. 1957. b y p e r m i s s i o n s.a. l o g o pedic society. 5. m o r g e n s t e r n , j . j . " p s y c h o l o g i c a l and social f a c t o r s in c h i l d r e n ' s s t a m m e r i n g . " p h . d . d i s s e r t a t i o n . univers i t y of e d i n b u r g h . 1953. (microfilm). r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the aristotelian mode of thought underlying research and theorizing in the field of stuttering* michael l. goodgoll, b.a. log. (rand) introduction it is the writer's contention that the difficulties inherent in the field of speech therapy have been underestimated and misunderstood. it is a common supposition that the problematic nature and outcome of therapy are caused by insufficient factual knowledge, and that the remedy is further research in order to build up our store of basic facts. it is believed that when this is accomplished, a creative theorist will arrive to conceptualize and organize this data into a comprehensive theory indicating the significance of, and the interrelationships between the facts as known, thereby providing a scientific basis for therapy. this leads to an acceptance of all research as being valuable in that, if nothing else, it contributes to our basic store of knowledge. for the speech therapist, both facts and theories are tools. theoretical understanding alone is insufficient; in addition the therapist has to apply his knowledge in actual therapy. it is essential, therefore, that the theories and facts he brings into the therapy situation be of the type that allows him to take cognizance of the individuality of each patient, in order to devise an effective therapeutic programme. it is the individuality of each case that, in the writer's opinion, is the crucial problem in speech therapy. in 1931 kurt lewin11 criticized contemporary psychology for its acceptance of an aristotelian mode of thought. lewin considered that this acceptance precluded our understanding of the individual as an individual. this paper will discuss lewin's criticisms in order to assess how applicable they are to the field of stuttering research and theory. by implication, a similar discussion could be held on other areas in speech therapy. / some characteristics of aristotelian thought aristotle considered that not all physical processes are lawful, and used the regularity or frequency of occurrence of an event as a criterion of lawfulness. only events certified by their repetitive nature as being lawful were considered to be conceptually intelligible and • b a s e d on a s t u d y p r o j e c t p r e s e n t e d ' t o t h e s u b d e p a r t m e n t of s p e e c h p a t h o l o g y a n d a u d i o l o g y , u n i v e r s i t y of t h e w i t w a t e r s r a n d , 1968. journal of the south african logopedi society, vol. 16, no. , dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e aristotelian mode 27 thus of scientific interest. individual events, i.e. those occurring only once or infrequently, were considered to be fortuitous or due to chance and therefore not lawful. it follows from this that for aristotle the individual event, the exception to the rule, does not constitute disproof of the rule. a further aspect of aristotelian thought is to consider that having defined a particular class of events because of the frequent occurrence of those events, one has also defined the essential nature of those events. according to aristotle, every object tends towards perfection and the realization of its own nature in so far as this tendency is not hindered or disturbed by outside forces. but, since this nature is for aristotle that which is common to the class of objects, we find that the class is both the concept and the goal of the object, i.e. it both defines the object and explains its behaviour. in summary, aristotelian thought has as maxims: (a) frequency implies lawfulness. (b) individual events are fortuitous. (c) exceptions to the rule are not counter-arguments. (d) class defines essence. group versus individual characteristics. lewin, in discussing the effects of aristotelian thought on research states:t h e fact that lawfulness and individuality are considered antitheses has two sorts of effect on actual research. it signifies in the first place a limitation of research. it makes it appear hopeless to try and understand the real, unique course of an emotion or the actual structure of a particular individual's personality. it thus reduces one to a treatment of these problems in terms of mere averages . . . it implies in addition to this limitation a certain laxity of research. (psychology) is satisfied with setting forth mere regularities. the demands of psychology on the stringency of its propositions go no further than to require a validity in general or on the average or as a rule." that this attitude is widespread in the field of stuttering is demonstrated by the following statements: west: does stuttering have one cause? i do not know, but its uniformity of manifestation from person to person would suggest a uniformity of etiology. t h e differences of stuttering p h e n o m e n a from case to case may be explained as individual variations due to physical, cultural, physiological, or even pathological deviations — factors independent of the syndrome of stuttering.1' bloodstein: . . . the complex life situations out of which stuttering grows can never exactly duplicate themselves from case to case. but it is clear that we can generalize about these situations, and a true explanation is achieved only by abstracting from them certain unvarying features . . . .3 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 28 μ. l. goodgoll eisenson: in spite of considerable individual variability, general tendencies could nevertheless be observed for stutterers taken together as a group. 5 freund: our above description of the processes underlying the pathogenesis of stuttering leaves open the enormous individual differences and tries to underline only the common features.1 the question to be asked is whether theories of stuttering based on an exclusion of individual differences, an emphasis on group tendencies and the abstraction of regularities across individuals, can form the basis for comprehending and integrating the unique characteristics of each stutterer. theorists appear to accept that concentrating on and selecting common features is all that is necessary for an understanding of stuttering. therefore, there is some reason to feel that on a theoretical level workers in the field of stuttering accept aristotle's dictum that regularity defines the sphere of scientific interest. it would, however, be incorrect to say that these same workers are insensitive to the importance of taking into account the individuality of each case. they all advocate in their therapeutic suggestions that the therapist must treat every case as an individual. in actuality it is the empathy and intuitive understanding of the therapist on which they rely for the recognition of individual differences and unique personality characteristics — these differences being considered somehow to transcend scientific analysis. faced with so many variations amongst stutterers, some theorists accept the eclectic view that stuttering can result from many different factors. however, this is merely a means of coping intellectually with the diversity. the therapist, faced with his individual case, is not given any indication which factors are important and to what degree. to say that all factors are important to various degrees is mere common sense. of what use then are the theories? research and theorizing have a reciprocal relationship — research stemming from theoretical issues and in turn providing data which must be included into a coherent theory. as an example of this relationship and as an exemplification of an acceptance' of the aristotelian approach, we will examin'e the fairly acceptable proposition: stuttering increases as the speaker is required to formulate the linguistic content. this statement was |made by eisenson5 as a result of reviewing a number of studies dealing with changes in the incidence and severity of stuttering in various speaking situations. one of the studies eisenson quotes is by newman, on the adaptation effect in two situations — reading and self-formulated speech. newman: : ! reported that stutterers had been shown to adapt to both reading and journal of the south african logopedie society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the aristotelian mode 29 spontaneous speech. no mention was made of any discrepant findings. it is on these results that eisenson bases his conclusions. however, some time later (nine years), newman14 reported that of his twenty subjects, six did not adapt in the reading situation and seven did not adapt in the self-formulation situation. the fact that individual stutterers showed in some cases opposite behaviours was not considered important enough to report; nor was it considered an invalidation of his conclusion. this is a clear example of how individual differences are ignored for the sake of conceptual neatness. yet it is theories based on this sort of evidence that form the context for decisions in therapy. the end result is that if the patient is typical and shows problems or symptoms in common with other stutterers, we feel secure in applying to him whatever theoretical understanding we have of the average stutterer. however, if he does not fit within the bounds of the average stutterer, and presents individual and unique characteristics (which is the rule rather than the exception) and if we rely for our understanding on the above type of theory, then we are forced to limit our understanding to what is not unique and individual about him. we are thus, in a sense, treating fictional stutterers. use of statistics. lewin, in discussing the effects of aristotelian thought on psychology, writes: the statistical procedure . . . is the most striking expression of this aristotelian mode of thinking. in order to exhibit the common features of a given group of facts, the average is calculated. this average acquires a representative value and is used to characterize . . . the properties of the . . . child.1 1 it is necessary to examine closely the part played by statistics in under-emphasizing individual characteristics — statistics by its very nature being concerned with properties that are descriptive of the group or aggregation itself, rather than with properties of particular members (ferguson"). j. g. taylor1 5'1" has severely criticized the use of experimental design as a palliative for the scientist's uneasiness concerning the logical consistency of psychological theories. it satisfies his desire for logical consistency in that he can test any hypothesis, even the most trivial, in a rigorous mathematical manner, and thereby avoid the problem of producing theories or explanations of general validity, which will enable us to understand the individual as an individual. the use of tests of significance of the difference between means has received specific criticism by r. bakan1 — their misuse indicating a deep-rooted acceptance of the aristotelian approach to psychology. bakan's criticism revolves around the confusion between induction to the aggregate and induction to the general. general statements apply to all members of the population to which they refer. a general tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 μ. l. goodgoll statement is therefore critically testable, since any exception is a threat to its validity. aggregate-type propositions refer to the class of members considered as a group, and are concerned with characteristics of the class and not any particular member of the class. in a typical experiment setting out to test an hypothesis concerning stuttering, a group of stutterers and a group of normals are tested under similar conditions. after the tests have been scored, and statistical calculations done on the data, it might be found that the means differ significantly at the 1% level of significance. what may legitimately be inferred from this result? — only that the mean of all normals is different from the mean of all stutterers on this test (accepting that the sample is random and reflects the characteristics of the population from which it was drawn). the test of significance does not relate to the characteristics of each member of the population. it is thus invalid to draw any conclusions as regards the characteristics of an individual stutterer. if the original hypothesis was concerned only with the characteristics of the class, then this statistical procedure would be valid. one example of this confusion is contained in johnson's well-known proposition that at the time of onset of stuttering it is not possible to differentiate between the child who stutters and the child who does not stutter by examining their speech behaviour. johnson10 compared the nonfluencies of a group of children thought to stutter and those of a group of non-stuttering children. his statistical analysis of the scores obtained attempted to answer the following questions: (a) what are the distributions of the nonfluency measures for the two groups respectively? (b) what difference in the nonfluency measures can be demonstrated between the two groups? (c) to what extent do the various distributions of nonfluency measures for the two groups overlap? johnson concludes that the question of whether or not a given child is or is not stuttering at any given moment cannot be answered by measuring or observing the nonfluency of his speech.w however, the question of whether or not a given child is or is not stuttering was not originally asked, and it is highly apparent that johnson's statistical treatment of his data, being concerned with group tendencies, does not allow for any conclusions regarding individual stutterers. much of the discussion centred on johnson's conclusions has been concerned with the following inference: johnson's attitude seems to be that because there is insufficient regularity! in the characteristics of stuttering-type nonfluencies across individuals, the nonfluencies themselves journal of the south african logopedie society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the aristotelian mode 31 are of little scientific importance. he therefore suggests that we direct our attention away from the child's speech to the child's environment. other workers have felt that the mere fact that we cannot classify the child according to his speech behaviour does not imply that a study of his individual speech pattern is not relevant to our understanding of stuttering. johnson's argument is thus based on an invalid statistical inference and an acceptance of the aristotelian maxim that regularity defines the sphere of scientific interest. a further experimental procedure is to test groups of stutterers under different conditions. again, a t-test might show significant differences in mean scores. here the experimenter can legitimately infer that the mean of the general population of stutterers would be lower or higher under condition one than under condition two. but this applies only to the specific task done under these two conditions, and not to the general conditions themselves. (bakan1). an illustration of this problem is brown's experiment showing that stutterers stutter mainly on nouns, verbs, adjectives and adverbs (grammatical factor). from his results brown4 made two inferences: (a) all stutterers would show this effect; (b) they would show this effect in all situations. these experimental findings have been generally accepted in the field, and used in support of hypotheses concerning the learned nature of stuttering. however, brown conducted his experiment with adult stutterers in a reading situation, and more recent experiments have shown that stuttering is not related in the same way to the grammatical function of words when measured in spontaneous speech (hejnas); nor is there a similar relationship in the speech of young children who stutter (bloodstein and gantwerk3). both of brown's inferences appear, therefore, to be incorrect. it is important to note that the restricted nature of brown's conclusions became apparent only more than two decades later when the above-ment'^ned two experiments were conducted. up to this time theorists and therapists had accepted that brown's conclusions were generally valid. this discussion is not intended as a blanket criticism of statistics. the point being made is that statistics are of little use in arriving at an understanding of the individual person and how, for instance, the many factors characterizing stuttering are integrated in one person. confusion of class and essence. johnson has frequently emphasized that we should be aware of the way we use language to convey reality. statements are at different levels of inference or abstraction. he has specifically pointed out the error of confusing descriptive tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 μ. l. goodgoll statements such as i repeat sounds or / stutter with animistic statements such as / am a stutterer or i repeat sounds because i am a stutterer.9 the latter two statements imply that there is some force or entity within the person which produces the overt behaviour we describe as stuttering. this confusion, according to johnson, results from semantic problems. it is also possible to see a deeper reason: abstracting common features of stuttering and then positing these common features as an explanation, is essentially the aristotelian maxim of identifying class with essence, i.e. what is common between objects or events is also the true nature of these events. the idea of a force underlying the behaviour of the class of events, stems from aristotle's identification of class and goal. developmental psychology has been criticized essentially for this fault. researchers dealing with norms of child behaviour or development tend to use the norms discovered through empirical investigation as explanatory devices. for instance, children are observed to be negativistic at a certain stage of development, and negativism is subsequently posited as the explanation for their behaviour. there are more subtle examples of this type of thinking. stuttering has been found to develop in stages, but it is invalid to explain a child's speech behaviour by saying he is, for example, a transitional stage stutterer. luper and mulder advise the following procedure in planning therapy: first of all, after reviewing available information, the examiner estimates the relative stage of development of the stuttering, for example he may decide that the child has reached phase two . . . . h e then considers recommended treatment procedures for that phase of stuttering. f o r example, the phase two stutterer needs to change some basic evaluations about himself as a speaker.1 2 luper and mulder thus accept that having classified the child as a phase two stutterer on the basis of how similar he is to the average phase two stutterer, he therefore is a phase two stutterer and can be treated on the basis of this classification. lewin writes: so long as one rewards as important and conceptually intelligible only such properties of an object as are common to a whole group of objects, the individual differences of degree remain without scientific relevance, for in the abstractly defined classes these differences more or less disappear." obviously it is necessary to have some conceptual framework by means of which to organize complex and diverse facts; but if one's conceptual framework is of the kind that screens out all hut what is common between individuals, it must preclude a true understanding of the individual. i the non-aristotelian approach having examined the thesis that aristotelian thinking underlies some aspects of research and theorizing on stuttering, we can now ask what a non-aristotelian approach implies. | ι journal of the south african logopedie society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the aristotelian mode 33 briefly, it consists of an acceptance of the following proposition: all events are lawful, independent of their frequency of occurrence. this leads to an extraordinary increase in the demands made upon proof. the exception becomes completely valid disproof of a general type proposition. as taylor writes: a genuine scientific law must account for the exception as well as the rule, and this implies that a successful experiment must involve a more detailed investigation of individual cases than is customary. t h e experimenter cannot rest content with a demonstration that the treatment produces the expected results in some subjects; he cannot have a true understanding of the laws of the system unless he can show that the same laws, operating on a subject who differs from the others in some particular respect, must produce different results.10 the criterion for acceptance of a theory within the non-aristotelian frame of thinking therefore lies in the ability of the theory to predict individual differences, when within the general terms of the theory we substitute values derived from an individual. taylor's statement has an important implication: the differences in subjects' responses in an experiment are understood by taking into account the individual characteristics of each subject. the fact that subjects respond differently can thus become proof, not disproof, of the law being investigated. taylor's masterly and definitive work the behavioural basis of perception17 exemplifies the type of theorizing and experimenttation which is possible in the field of psychology. an acceptance of the above proposition also implies that processes or events not occurring regularly or frequently are valid fields of scientific investigation. the unusual stutterer, who presents atypical characteristics, or the child with an unusual type of speech pattern, are of great importance since they might allow us to discern most clearly how stuttering is constituted in one individual. therefore, in spite of the fact that it is unlikely that the experimenter would find a similar case, the detailed study of such individual cases is of scientific importance. taylor1'1 has argued that a psychological theory should not consist of a catalogue of variables in terms of their frequency of occurrence, but should explain how these variables hang together in the individual. it is this approach to the problem of stuttering that the writer considers will offer us the greatest possibility of attaining an understanding of the individual stutterer. summary as a direct result of the prevalence of aristotelian thinking in the field of speech pathology, research and theorizing has tended to ignore the individual, his unique characteristics and differences, and tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 μ. l. goodgoll to concentrate on what is common between individuals. the therapist is therefore faced with being unable to utilize effectively any particular theory, since none of them explains the unique attributes of the individual case with which he has to deal. a non-aristotelian approach to research on stuttering seems to hold more promise of attaining an eventual understanding of the individual stutterer. opsomming as gevolg van die oorheersing van die aristoteliaanse denkwyse op die gebied van die spraakpatologie, vind ons dat die individu met sy unieke eienskappe en verskille, misken word deur navorsers en teoretici. daar word meer gekonsentreer op wat algemeen is in verskillende individue. die terapeut vind dit moeilik, of selfs onmoontlik om effektief gebruik te maak van enige teorie, aangesien nie een die unieke kenmerke van die individu wat sy behandel, bespreek nie. dit blyk dat 'n nie-aristoteliaanse benadering vir navorsing oor hakkel ons nader sal bring aan die uiteindelike kennis van die individuele hakkelaar. references 1. bakan. r. (1967): on method: toward a reconstruction of psychological investigation. san francisco, josse-bass inc. 2. bloodstein, o. q958): stuttering as an anticipatory struggle reaction. stuttering: a symposium (ed.) j. eisenson. new york, h a r p e r and bros., pp. 1-69. 3. bloodstein, o. and gantwerk, b. f. (1967): grammatical function in relation to stuttering in young children. journal of speech and hearing research, 10, 786-789. 4. brown, s. f. (1937): the influence of grammatical function on the incidence of stuttering. journal of speech disorders, 2, 207-215. 5. eisenson, j. (1968): a perseverative theory of stuttering. stuttering: a symposium, (ed.) j. eisenson. new york, h a r p e r and bros., pp. 223-271. 6. ferguson, g. a. (1966): statistical analysis in psychology and education. (2nd ed.) mcgraw-hill. 7. freund, h. (1966): psychopathology and the problems of stuttering. illinois, charles c. thomas. 8. hejna, r. f. (1955): a study of the loci of stuttering in spontaneous speech. dissertation abstracts, 15 1674-1675 9. johnson, w. (1958): introduction: the six men and the stuttering in stuttering: a symposium (ed.) j. eisenson, new york, harper and bros., pp. xi-xxiv. / 10. johnson, w. (1959): the onset of stuttering. minneapolis, university of minnesota press. 11. lewin, k . (1931): the conflict between aristotelian and galileian 'modes of thought in contemporary psychology. journal of general psychology, 5, 141-177. 12. luper, h . l. and mulder, r. l. (1964): stuttering therapy for children. new jersey, prentice hall. 13. n e w m a n , p. w. (1954): a study of the adaptation and recovery of the stuttering response in self-formulated \ speech. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 19, 450-458. 1 journal of the south african logopedie society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e aristotelian mode . . . 3 5 14. n e w m a n , p. w. (1963): adaptation performance of individual stutterers: implication for research. journal of speech and hearing research, 6, 293-294. 15. taylor, j. g. (1958): scientific method in psychology iv. british journal of statistical psychology, 11, 133-] 35. 16. taylor, j. g. (1958): experimental design: a cloak for intellectual sterility. british journal of psychology, 49, 106-116. 17. taylor, j. g. (1962): the behavioural basis of perception. n e w haven, yale university press. 18. west. r. (1958): an agnostic's speculations about stuttering. stuttering: a symposium, (ed.) j. eisenson. n e w york, h a r p e r and bros., pp. 167-222. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a n o t e on pitch control in e s o p h a g e a l speech a. traill ( m . l i t t . ( e d i n ) ) department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. summary an unusual case of esophageal pitch control first discussed in this journal is re-examined in the light of additional data. it is claimed that high-pitched stressed vowels show evidence of diplophonia, a possibility not described in earlier studies of this case. opsomming 'n buitengewone geval van esofagale toonhoogte kontrole wat van te vore' in hierdie tydskrif bespreek is, is weereens ondersoek in die lig van addisionele gegewens. daar word beweer dat hoe toonhoogte klem-vokale tekens toon van diplofonie, 'n moontlikheid wat nie vroeer in hierdie geval beskryf is nie. in two p a p e r s , 1 ' 2 published in this journal, l. w. lanham and w. a. kerr (lk) discuss a case, mrs b. mcl, who had two modes of esophageal phonation, a low-pitched mode and a high-pitched mode. the case is noteworthy not only because of the impression created during speech of wide pitch variation (so much so that it is known to have deceived . . . even the most experienced ear as to the true nature of the voice),2 but also because the high-pitched mode of phonation is rare amongst laryngectomees. lk claim that the two modes of phonation alternate in speech, and that the transitions from one to the other are abrupt. the mechanism for pitch regulation therefore does not involve a continuous variation of tension in the esophageal sphincter but a switch from one vibratory source to another. it is of interest to learn that the esophageal sphincter is the vibratory source for both high and low-pitched phonation. lk's figures d2 and d3 from their earlier paper (reproduced below as figures 1 and 2) illustrate the two adjustments of the sphincter for the lowand high-pitched modes of phonation respectively. the acoustic output of the configuration in figure 1 is described by lk1 as consisting of . . . vibrations of low frequency in the form of noise bursts lacking a harmonic structure . . . and that of figure 2 as consisting of . . . pressure pulses with a harmonic structure and true frequency modulation . . . the purpose of this note is to present additional data from this case which suggest that lk's account is incomplete. the acoustic evidence presented below indicates firstly that both modes of phonation may take place at the same time and secondly (indeed as evidence for the ' first claim) that both modes may produce pulses with harmonics. this complex mode of phonation thus constitutes a third type. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 96 a. traill figure 1. lk's figure d2 showing the cricopharyngeal articulation during low-pitched esophageal phonation. fig d 3 figure 2. lk's figure d3 showing the cricopharyngeal articulation during high-pitched esophageal phonation. this investigation was prompted by a recording of conversational speech produced by mrs mcl. an auditorily prominent feature of the pitch variation was that a particularly high pitch appeared with long stressed vowels, as part of "expressive intonation". the "voice" quality at these points was distinctive, giving an impression of being "squeezed" and mildly harsh. scale magnified spectrograms were made of 20 sees, of speech in order to establish the acoustic nature of this peculiar auditory effect. in almost every case of these auditorilydetermined pitch prominences, there was evidence of diplophonia. figure 3 illustrates the relevant sections of the spectrograms. at the points marked with arrows there are unrelated harmonics. figure 3(a) shows clearly how a harmonic at about 1,28 khz varies in pitch independently of the one at about 0,43 khz which remains,, largely level. in figure 3(b) and (c) one can see pitch differences between two pairs of successive harmonics, one pair, rising and falling in pitch while the other pair remains stationary. these unrelated pairs are therefore not harmonics of the same fundamental frequency (fo). unrelated harmonics that vary in pitch in these ways are readily detectable. however, it is obviously not necessary that unrelated harmonics should show precisely these kinds of pitch variation; they may in fact have the same pitch pattern and these patterns may be level. in such cases mere inspection is not sufficient to determine relatedness of harmonics and the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pitch control in esophageal speech 97 it is necessary to calculate whether they could be related to fo and to one another. k h z ( a ) ( b ) ( c ) ( d ) figure 3. narrow band scale-magnified spectrograms of four utterances produced by mrs mcl showing diplophonia. the unrelated harmonics are marked with arrowheads. figure 3(d) has four clear harmonics with frequencies of 320 hz, 640 hz, 1 610 hz and 1 720 hz at the point opposite the arrow on the baseline. visual inspection of the different pitch intervals between the higher pair and between the lower pair suggests a possible anomaly. assuming that the lowest harmonic is the fundamental, then successive higher harmonics are expected at 640 hz, 960 hz, 1 280 hz, 1 600 hz, 1 920 hz etc. the second, harmonic occurs at the expected frequency and the next visible one is the lower of the two higher ones at 1 610 hz. allowing for a margin of error for manual measurement, this will be the fifth harmonic of fo = 320 hz (320 hz x 5 = 1 600 hz). the next harmonic in this series will occur at 1 920 hz, thus showing that the second of the two higher harmonics at 1 720 hz is not part of the series and must have an independent fo. put differently, the vibratory source producing this harmonic is not the same as the one producing the others. before leaving this point we should consider the possibility that the relationship between the higher pair of harmonics is actually being obscured by the fact that a number of harmonics are of such weak intensity that they are not visible. for instance assuming fo = 70 hz, the fifth harmonic will occur at 350 hz, the ninth at 630 hz, the 23rd at 1 610 hz and the 25th at 1 750 hz. once again, the margin of error could account for the discrepancies between these frequencies and those observed in 3(d) and, of course, one would have to assume that die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 98 a. traill fo was not visible. one might thus be tempted to challenge the claim that 3(d) shows evidence of diplophonia. however, this would be incorrect for two reasons. firstly, a range of 25 harmonics is never found in the acoustic record of this case; nine to ten harmonics seems to be the limit. secondly, suggestion that there is a weak fo at 70 hz is contrary to the perception of a high-pitched fo at that point. we may safely accept therefore that 3(d) does show diplophonia. it is of interest to note that in those cases where the fundamental frequency of the unrelated harmonics is clear, (figs. 3(b) (c)), it is relatively high for both. therefore both harmonics may presumably be identified with the crico/pharyngeal adjustment in figure 2, that is, for mrs mcl's high-pitched phonation. of course, it cannot be ruled out that a third configuration could have existed corresponding to the cases of diplophonia. diplophonia is usually regarded as a bizarre or abnormal type of phonation when the vocal folds are involved. however, in mrs mcl's case this evaluation is hardly appropriate. the diplophonia adds to the perceived range of expressive pitch variation which makes her speech so unusual and satisfactory. it is worth asking whether there is anything distinctive anatomically that could produce so flexible a vibratory source. l k 2 claim that mrs mcl's . . . pseudoglottal vibration is the product of a relatively intact musculature and considerable effort . . . and, as has been noted, they locate it at the site of the cricopharyngeal sphincter in (fig. 2). they discuss a number of muscular adjustments that distinguish the shape of this sphincter in high-pitched and low-pitched phonation. in their earlier paper they also refer to a pair of antero-posterior folds in the hypopharynx which they claim are . . . in apposition during pharyngeal phonation. the latter possibility is not mentioned again in the later paper. the anatomical picture is therefore complex and there is no way to relate it precisely to the acoustic output. however, it may be worth speculating that the "relatively intact musculature" lk refer to could be expected to play a crucial role in producing this unusual type of esophageal pitch control. references 1. w. a. kerr and l. w. lanham. (1973): anatomical and spectrographic analysis of the voice in disease: a report of five cases. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, 20, 1, pp, 81-105. j 2. l. w. lanham and w. a. kerr. (1975): pitch in esophageal speech. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, 22, pp. 31-41. j the south african journal o ommunication disorders, vol. 27, 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) r e p u b l i c h e a r i n g c o n s u l t a n t s cptyj a i d l t d , 7 3 2 medical c i t y e l o f f cor. jeppe s t r e e t johannesburg, transvaal t e l e p h o n e 2 3 6 6 8 5 p . o . 8 o x 5 2 0 4 1 s a x o n w o l d 2 1 3 2 hearing aids. we specialize in the supply and fitting of hearing aids for all hearing losses, especially for nerve deafness recruitment bone conduction cases. cross aids: cros bicros multicros etc. bone conduction aids for body, earlevel, glasses (speciality by viennatone) binaural fittings we import and stock : — viennatone, qualitone, microson, phonak hearing aids. moulds: soft, hard, skeleton, vented, occluded etc. repairs: all aids supplied with a scientific performance report after repair. special prices for dealers and institutions. accessories: teacher — pupil, parent child, individual audiotrainers, very reasonably priced. group audio trainers. tv — wireless infrared transistor receiver sets made by sennheiser. audiometers: screening, diagnostic, research, era and electrocochleography c.o.r. and peep show. impedance bridges. manufacturers of sound proof booth and sound proof rooms. hearing aid testing set by " f o n i x " u.s.a. phonak noise generator with different frequencies, pure tone and warble tone, for everyday's use. we repair and calibrate audiometers. sound level meters, calibrators. industrial noise consultants. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 27,· 1980 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society editorial in the last edition of the journal we were able to read articles dealing with some of the many types of defects handled by the logopodecian. in this issue of the journal, there are three articles on language disorders in children. during the past decade an increasing interest in the study of language and its disorders has been shown. one of the findings resulting from this growing interest is that if the language function is disturbed in any way, difficulties ranging from complete lack of speech to a deficiency in reading can be found. since reading is an important aspect of the language functwn, an article on the teaching of reading to the cerebral palsied child by ε. m. harrison is presented in this issue; isaac jolles presents his views on the teaching of language and conceptual thinking to the brain-injured child, and an interesting case history of an aphasic child, compiled jointly by dr. law and mrs. shavell concludes thai section of the journal in which the emphasis has been on language disorders. an article on organic dyslalia by a. rummel is indicative of the fact that the often so-called dull subject of dyslalia, when correctly handled, can make interesting reading and stimulate one to think of dyslalia from many new and different angles. berea cycle works toys and cycles cycles for hire 59 kotze street, hillbrow, johannesburg. phone 44-1214 we specialize in educational toys r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) editorial it is with pride that we present the first bilingual issue of the journal of the south african logopedic society. the first students are to graduate, in the coming year, from the speech therapy department at the university of pretoria, where the course is conducted in afrikaans. in anticipation of this we feel that it is fitting that this journal should contain the two official languages of south africa. it is significant that all but one of the contributions to this issue of the journal are from south africans. in the past we have relied upon the contribution of articles from overseas authorities, but in this issue we have attempted to reflect that speech therapy in south africa is reaching a stage where it too can make a contribution to the field. original research is increasing with a greater number of people taking postgraduate degrees. we hope that in the near future we shall be able to publish the results of these research projects. for the present, however, we have attempted to give a glimpse of the work that is being done. as such, the articles contributed by speech therapists are of a more practical nature, while the contributions from our medical practitioners relate to those spheres of medical science which have direct bearing on the work of speech therapists. it is, therefore, with pleasure that we present the december 1962 issue of the journal of the south african logopedic society, and hope that the wide variety of subjects covered by our contributors, will provide something of interest to every reader. december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 3 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) redaksioneel dit is met trots dat ons die eerste tweetalige uitgawe van die tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse spraakheelkundige vereniging aanbied. die eerste studente van die universiteit van pretoria, waar die kursus in afrikaans gedoseer word, gradueer in 1963. na aanleiding hiervan meen ons dat dit gepas is dat die tydskrif in albei die landstale van suid-afrika gedruk word. dit is opmerklik dat al die bydraes, behalwe een, in die tydskrif van suidafrikaners kom. in die verlede het ons staat gemaak op bydraes van oorsese gesaghebbendes, maar in die uitgawe het ons gepoog om aan te toon dat spraakheelkunde in suid-afrika die stadium bereik het om ook 'n bydrae te lewer tot spraakheelkunde as 'n wetenskap. aangesien meer terapeute nagraadse studie onderneem, word belangrike oorspronklike navorsing gedoen. ons hoop om in die nabye toekoms die resultate van hierdie navorsingskemas te publiseer. ons wil egter nou net 'n beeld skep van die werk wat gedoen word. dus is die artikels, bydraes deur die spraakterapeute, van meer praktiese belang, terwyl die bydraes van ons mediese praktisyns in verband staan met daardie aspekte van die mediese wetenskap wat van direkte belang is vir die spraakterapeute. dit is dus 'n voorreg om die desember 1962 uitgawe van die tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse spraakheelkundige vereniging aan te bied, en ons is vertrou dat die groot verskeidenheid van onderwerpe wat bespreek word iets van belang vir elke leser sal verskaf. journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an investigation of some aspects of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic subjects lesley caplan, b.a. (sp. & η. th.) (witwatersrand) speech therapy department, groote schuur hospital, cape town summary some dimensions of stuttering-like s y m p t o m s of five dysphasic patients were considered and c o m p a r e d t o several aspects of stuttering w h i c h are generally w e l l k n o w n and d o c u m e n t e d . the nature and a m o u n t of the dysfluencies of dysphasia were e x a m i n e d , and, although the a m o u n t o f non-fluency appeared t o justify the label of stuttering, the nature of the dysfluencies was m u c h like that observed in normal speakers and not that considered t o b e the distinguishing features of stuttering. the loci of t h e dysfluencies in the sequence o f dysphasic speech were investigated and the majority of subjects were found t o experience more difficulty o n the function words o f language rather than o n the c o n t e n t or lexical words which precipitate d y s f l u e n c y in stutterers. all subjects experienced the greatest difficulty o n words in the initial p o s i t i o n in the sentence as is found w i t h stutterers. for m o s t of the subjects the frequency o f dysfluency was highest o n longer words and it was observed that subjects generally experienced more difficulty on c o n s o n a n t s than o n vowels. under c o n d i t i o n s of propositionality it s e e m e d that there was s o m e increase in the frequency and severity of the dysfluencies of dysphasics w h i l e the adaptation task y i e l d e d divergent results. opsomming sommige aspekte van s i m p t o m e , soortgelyk aan hakkel, b y vyf disfasiepasiente, is m e t verskeie algemeen b e k e n d e en reeds b e w y s d e kenmerke van hakkel vergelyk. die aard en h o e v e e l h e i d o n v l o t h e d e van die disfasie-pasiente is ondersoek en a l h o e w e l die h o e v e e l h e i d o n v l o t h e d e die kenteken van hakkel regverdig, was die aard van die o n v l o t h e d e in 'n groot m a t e soortgelyk aan die o n v l o t h e d e wat b y normaalsprekendes v o o r k o m . die o n v l o t h e d e is dus nie verwant aan die onderskeidende kenmerke van hakkel nie. die spesifieke posisie van o n v l o t h e d e in die opeenvolging van die spraak van disfasie-pasiente is ondersoek. die meerderheid van die proefpersone het groter p r o b l e m e o p funksiewoorde van die taal ondervind en nie soseer prob l e m e gehad m e t die i n h o u d of leksikale w o o r d e soos hakkelaars nie. s o o s gevind b y hakkelaars, het al die proefpersone die grootste mate van moeilikh e d e ondervind m e t w o o r d e in die inisiele posisie van die sin. die frekwensie van die o n v l o t h e d e was o o k hoer b y die meerderheid van die proefpersone. daar is o o k waargeneem dat daar oor die algemeen meer p r o b l e m e o p konsonante as o p ander w o o r d e ondervind w o r d . dit het voorgekom asof 'n t o e n a m e in die frekwensie en erns van die o n v l o t h e d e b y die disfasie-gevalle ontstaan onder t o e s t a n d e van proposisionering, terwyl die aanpassingstaak uiteenlopende resultate gelewer het. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s o m e aspects of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic subjects 53 in many instances in the literature mention has been made of brain-injured adults, who have stuttering-like speech. various authorities cite cases of aphasia showing symptoms suggestive of stuttering and make reference to the repetition of sounds and syllables, hesitancies and the use of superfluous words accompanied by tension, in dysphasic speech. in attempting to explain the association of stuttering and aphasia writers have provided phrases such as "dysphasia with stuttering" (oltuszewski), "aphatic stuttering" (kussmaul), and "stuttering in aphasia" (froeschels)1. various pathologies of the central nervous system like parkinsonism, the extrapyramidal dysarthrias, as well as a number of pyramidal and cortical dysarthrias present symptoms that resemble stuttering — retarded rate of speech utterance that may be irregular and jerky, defective intonation, and a tendency towards perseveration where there may be a difficulty in the production of initial sounds3·6. in considering the field of stuttering etiology and symptomatology, theories and descriptions are in conflict with each other. it was st. onge2 0 who recommended the use of the "syndrome" concept to distinguish different types of stutterers in an attempt to eliminate some of this confusion. he propounded an approach directed towards isolating the particular symptoms of stuttering into consistent and meaningful entities, rather than viewing the disorder indiscriminately, as a mixture of psychological, motor and other signs. he suggests the separation of stutterers into three groups. i) the stutterer with no indication of constitutional or psychogenic symptoms, who simply manifests a phobia about speech. ii) the psychogenic stutterer who employs stuttering as a symptom in a more extensive disturbance. iii) the organic stutterer who manifests intolerance of non-fluency as a result of some structural abberation. it is this last alternative that bears relevance to the present study. in reviewing the literature on stuttering, various authorities have put forward a concept of stuttering as a sub-clinical aphasia. travis21 expounded a theory of cerebral dominance, karlin12, a psychosomatic theory of a delay in myelinization, west25 wrote of stuttering and epilepsy and eisenson7, a perseverative theory that stressed a neurological basis for stuttering. attention has been briefly focused above on these writers in the field of aphasia who make reference to stuttering-like symptoms, and, on authorities in the field of stuttering who, in turn, stress neurological factors. mention should also be made of the research by goldman-eisler and others who have shown that normal speakers too, manifest dysfluency at specific loci in the speech sequence and that this dysfluency is an integral part of the speech process2·2 2. it seems pertinent then to question and consider the relationship between stuttering and aphasia. one may approach this from the etiological point of view, but in this study, the writer has chosen to examine symptomologically tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 5 4 lesley caplan some dimensions of the stuttering-like speech of dysphasics, and to compare them with some dimensions of stuttered speech as reported in the literature. method an attempt will be made to examine the repetitive pattern of dysphasic speech in terms of the amount and nature of dysfluency; further, the loci of dysfluencies in the sequence of dysphasic speech will be investigated. an attempt will also be made to determine whether the dysfluencies of dysphasia are susceptible to the influences that bring about changes in stuttering behaviour. it is hypothesized that under conditions of propositionality, i.e. where communicative stress and meaningfulness of the presenting situation is increased, dysphasics will show an increase in the frequency and severity of dysfluency, while on the adaptation task where there are repeated sayings of the same phrases, a reduction of stuttering symptoms will manifest itself. subjects five adult aphasic patients, three males and two females were used as experimental subjects, one male and one female being afrikaans speaking. all subjects were classified by their speech therapists as predominantly expressive aphasics23, who were able to communicate spontaneously and meaningfully. an important criterion for selection was that the subjects should manifest some degree of non-fluency in their speech. the subjects also showed a variety of specific language disturbances, the most notable being anomia and apraxia. the subjects ranged in age from 41 to 62 years. no experimental group of stutterers was used as the symptomotology and the modification of stuttering behaviour has been extensively documented. procedure (a) spontaneous speech samples. johnson1 0 indicates that taperecorded samples of speech from adults can be obtained by using the "job" task, where the subject is encouraged to talk for 2-3 minutes on his chosen, likely or future vocation. a modification was introduced, in view of the emotionality of some subjects as regards their occupations, and topics such as daily activities, hobbies, family set-up and in some cases, vocations were suggested. / to obtain a second speaking sample, a picture from the thematic apperception test (tat) was used. johnson1 1 suggested the use of card no. 10 of the tat where the subject is asked to tell a story based on the picture, and points out that although the tat task might be more emotionally loaded than the "job" task, the type of speech samples elicited are very similar. the spontaneous speech samples were used.to provide data on the nature and amount of dysfluency and the loci of dysfluencies in the sequence of dysphasic speech: spencer brown6 was probably the most prominent journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, deceber 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some aspects of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic subjects 55 investigator in this field. he isolated four factors that he felt precipitate stuttering. he postulated that the grammatical function of words, their position in the sentence, the length of words and the phonetic characteristics of the initial sounds of the words, were important in evoking stuttering. it seems interesting to determine whether these factors are also important in accounting for the dysfluencies of dysphasia. (b) propositional speech sample. in view of the fact that propositionality appears to be a factor common to both aphasia and stuttering7'9 and because various authorities, particularly eisenson7, have drawn attention to the variability and inconsistency of stuttering and related it to the degree to which the stutterer is attempting to convey meaning, it was important to include this phenomenon in the experimental design. a test was devised to produce conditions of propositionality. it was based partly on an idea put forward by newman16 in his article on adaptation in self-formulated speech and partly on a procedure being used by the department of phonetics and general linguistics in collaboration with the engineering faculty at the university of the witwatersrand13 in teaching engineering students the art of communication. the task involves the recorded description of an arrangement of geometrical figures (fig. 1). the drawing used was based on the series of figures lanham1 3 described in his course for engineering students. the subjects were instructed as newman16 suggested to describe the drawing in such a way that a listener could make an accurate reproduction of the figure solely on the basis of the recorded directions. to increase communicative responsibility, it was pointed out that the recording would be played to first year students to determine their ability to follow directions. figure 1. diagram used t o elicit a propositional speech sample. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 lesley caplan (c) adaptation task. a definition of adaptation relevent to this study is one postulated by schaef19, where he considers adaptation as " . . . . the reduction of the frequency of the stuttering response with repeated and continued elicitation of verbal behaviour under constant stimulus conditions". in response to st. onge's20 plea for the differentiation of stutterers into various clinical syndromes, newman16 postulates the use of the adaptation effect as a "discriminating tool". this provides then, a rationale for the employment of this phenomenon. it seems that an indication of the adaptation trends of aphasic patients may well provide a model of the adaptation trend to be expected in the organic stutterer. in this study adaptation was determined in two ways. in the first, a questionand-answer technique devised by schaef19 for stutterers was used. five questions were asked, and each question had to be answered using the stem of the question in the answer; e.g. "what city is this?" "this city is ". five questions constituted one trial, and each subject was given three consecutive trials, where the same questions were asked and the same answers given. the second involved repetition of five sentences selected from "arthur the young rat" on three consecutive trials11. the number of sentences was arbitrarily selected on the basis of the assumed capabilities of the most severe aphasic subject. (d) analysis of the spontaneous speech sample. a 150-word segment of speech was selected from the spontaneous speech sample elicited by using johnson's "job" and tat tasks. the criteria for selection of the 150-word segment were that the particular segment should not be interrupted by any unduly long pauses and that it should comprise portions of both the "job" and tat tasks. this 150-word segment was analysed in terms of the following factors:the nature of the dysfluencies. the dysfluencies were identified and categorized from a transcript made of the relevant segments of the speech sample. the categories were those suggested by johnson1 0 and are: interjections of sounds, syllables, words and phrases; part-word repetitions; word repetitions; phrase repetitions; revisions; incomplete phrases; broken words; prolonged sounds. the severity of the dysfluencies. for this purpose three measures were used: i) the rate of utterance: this dimension was chosen because it is considered to be a measure of stuttering independent of listener evaluations and, therefore, one of the most objective methods available. it is cbmputed in words per minute and represents the ratio of verbal output to speaking t i m e 1 0 · 1 1 . ii) a rating scale of the severity of dysfluency was used. experimentation has shown that a 9-point equal-appearing interval scale of severity can have internal consistency and is reliable27. three judges who were practicing speech therapists, rated the severity of the dysfluencies. iii) the frequency of stuttering as computed of the total dysfluency index also gave some indication of the severity of the dysfluencies in the journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, decetnber 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s o m e aspects of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic subjects 5 7 speech samples. this represents the most comprehensive dysfluency analysis where all dysfluencies in the speech sample are identified, classified and tabulated. the sum of the category index constitutes this measure. the following formula was used for the computation: tdi = (n/a) 100 where η = total number of dysfluencies, and a = verbal o u t p u t 1 1 . the loci of the dysfluencies. i) grammatical function of words: having analysed the nature and type of the dysfluencies in the speech sample, every word that was considered to be dysfluent was tabulated in terms of its grammatical function in the speech sequence. the parts of speech considered were nouns, verbs, adjectives, adverbs, articles, prepositions, pronouns and conjunctions. in addition the total number of each of these parts of speech in the sample was enumerated, e.g. the number of dysfluent nouns was determined as well as the total number of nouns in the speech sample, and the ratio of dysfluent nouns to the total number of nouns was presented as a percentage. ii) the phonetic factor: again having tabulated all the dysfluent words as described above, the initial sounds were tabulated and the percentage of words beginning with a particular sound out of the total number of words beginning with that sound in the speech sample, was computed. the total number of dysfluent consonants and the total number of dysfluent vowels were counted and represented a percentage of the total number of consonants and vowels in the entire speech sample for each subject respectively. iii) position of word in the sentence: the position of the words identified as dysfluent in the sentence was determined. the total number of words that occurred first, second, third etc; in the sentences in the speech sample were also counted in order to find out what percentage of the first, second, third etc., words in the sentences were dysfluent. iv) word length: the same procedure as described above was applied in the investigation of this factor. again the length of each dysfluent word was determined and the number of words of a particular length were coupled as a percentage of the total number of words of that particular length in the entire speech sample. (e) analysis of the adaptation task. the responses obtained, employing the question-and-answer technique19 and repetitions of sentences from "arthur the young rat" were subjected to frequency counts. the number of words considered to be dysfluent on the first, second and third trials were numbered and presented as a percentage of the total number of words uttered in each trial. adaptation was computed using the following formula: (a-b)/a where a and b were the number of words identified as dysfluent on trials one and two, for example. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 5 8 l e s l e y caplan (f) subjective comments by judges on the speech samples. prior to being asked to rate the severity of dysfluencies in the speech samples, the judges were asked to "comment on" the speech samples. the judges were completely unaware of the objectives of this study project and unfamiliar with the subjects used in the study. the comments were considered in terms of what the judges believed the speech problems of the subjects were. results and discussion an attempt will be made to discuss any of the similarities or differences found in the responses of dysphasics when compared with the findings on stuttering as presented in the literature. 1. the amount, nature and loci of dysfluencies. the data for the amount of the dysfluencies of dysphasia and their nature were obtained from the sample of spontaneous speech. it is interesting to note the total dysfluency index for each subject and to compare this with the norms given by johnson1 1 in determining whether or not the amount of dysfluency justifies labelling the aphasic "a stutterer". despite the wide range of dysfluencies manifested by both stutterers and non-stutterers, it seems that three of the five subjects in this study could possibly have been considered stutterers (table 1). however the nature of the dysfluency in dysphasic speech and the nature of stuttering appear to differ. in order to identify the dysfluencies in the speech of the dysphasics and to differentiate them from moments of stuttering, the eight categories suggested by johnson1 0 were employed. subjects si. s2. s3. s4. s5. t o t a l d y s f l u e n c y i n d e x 34 16 27,33 14 38,67 nature of the dysfluen cies interj e c t i o n s 16 5,3 11,33 3,33 18 nature of the dysfluen cies part-word r e p e t i t i o n 4 ,67 1,33 1,33 7,33 / nature of the dysfluen cies word r e p e t i t i o n 3,33 2,(57 9,33 1,33 6,67 nature of the dysfluen cies revisions 4,6 3,33 1,33 2 3,33 table i. percentage of dysfluencies and the nature of dysfluencies in a 150-word sample of the speech of dysphasic subjects. journal of the south african speech a hearing association vol. 19, deceber 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some a s p e c t s of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic subjects 59 all subjects manifested the greatest number of dysfluencies in the category of interjections. the dysfluencies categorized as word-repetitions, part-word repetitions, and revisions also occurred frequently, and to a much lesser extent than interjections (table 1). it is important to note that the disorder of stuttering encompasses much more than moments of dysfluency and that dysfluency per se does not constitute the whole of the problem known as stuttering11. in view of the information already cited, it appears that the non-fluencies shown by the subjects in this experiment are similar to the non-fluencies exhibited by normal speakers, and not those considered to be the distinguishing features of stuttering10. from the results then, two apparently conflicting aspects emerged, and in order to determine whether the aphasic subjects did, in fact, appear to "sound" like stutterers, three judges were invited to comment on the speech samples. as mentioned, these judges were qualified speech therapists who were not aware of the speech and language problems of the subjects nor of the objectives of this study. the judges drew attention to patterns such as hesitancy and repetitions in speech, use of interjections as anti-expectancy devices, circumlocutions as well as manifestations of anxiety and tension. generally it seems, that on the basis of trained listener evaluations, symptoms that have been regarded as peculiar to dysphasia, (anomia, incomplete sentences, sequencing difficulties) can be re-interpreted and viewed within the framework of stuttering behaviour. it seems plausible that other factors (apart from those mentioned) like anxiety and tension, as well as disturbances in spontaneity of expression, could be significant in contributing to the evaluation of stuttering. the amount of dysfluency manifested by the subjects appeared to justify the label of stuttering while the nature of the dysfluencies seem to be that revealed in the speech of normal speakers and not that which many authorities consider to be the distinguishing features of stuttering. despite this, the findings should not be considered an impasse and the comments of the judges in evaluating the speech as stuttered speech emphasizes this point. it appears that here are other more subtle attributes of the dysfluencies that contribute to the diagnosis of stuttering. the sample of spontaneous speech obtained in the "job" and tat tasks yielded the data for the analysis of the loci of dysfluencies. the grammatical function of words. the majority of subjects produced results contrary to those expected from stutterers. more difficulty was experienced in the function words of language (prepositions, articles, pronouns and conjunctions) rather than on the content or lexical words (nouns, adjective, adverbs, and verbs). the postulate that stuttering tends to occur on those words that are focal points in the speech sequence, i.e. where the transmission of meaning is most important, and where the speaker's emphasis and the interest of the listener is concentrated, is not demonstrated by the findings with the dysphasic cases in this study. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheekunde vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 0 l e s l e y caplan it is interesting to examine these results in relation to the data on the loci of stuttering in the speech sequence of primary stutterers, as reported in the literature. bloodstein2 indicates that in phase 1 stutterers, stuttering occurs on the "small" parts of speech — the pronouns, conjunctions and prepositions and a modification occurs with the development of stuttering. it is precisely on these "small" parts of speech that the dysphasic subjects had difficulty. weiss's24 concept of central language imbalance, or cluttering, is also worth ' considering as it seems that the initial stage of stuttering and that of cluttering have been described as identical and that the development of this "imbalance in speech" into stuttering is dependent on the reactions of the individual to the primary disorder. it is possible, therefore, to assume that.in dysphasia there is a situation comparable with the postulated undifferentiated primary stages of stuttering and cluttering. word length. in considering the effect of word length (as calculated by number of letters) on the frequency and severity of dysfluencies in dysphasic speech, the greatest frequency of occurrence of dysfluencies was found on the longer words for four of the five subjects: dysfluencies occurred 100% of the time on the longest words and for one subject the second longest word elicited the most dysfluency. these results were confounded to some extent, by the occasional occurrence of severe dysfluency on short words and minimum dysfluency on longer words. wingate26 has explained the finding that longer words tend to evoke more dysfluency, in terms of the fact that longer words involve a more intricate pattern of motor co-ordination. this has neurological implications. position of the word in the sentence. the most prominent characteristic of the data here, is the fact that all five subjects showed the greatest frequency of dysfluency on the words in initial position in the sentence. the percentage of dysfluency on initial words was 27% in one case, 16% in another, and in three of the cases at least 10% greater than the scores obtained on the words in other positions. these results approximate those observed in stuttering, where there is "a gradient of stuttering increasing with proximity to the initial words". (quarrington, conway & siegel17). the phonetic factor. in observing the results obtained in determining the influence of the phonetic factor in precipitating dysfluency, it was obvious that this was a markedly variable factor, differing from individual to individual. no particular sound could be determined to precipitate dysfluency, but it was observed that subjects experienced more difficulty on initial consonants than on initial vowels. this tendency has been described in the literature on stuttering. it seems that consonants involve more tension of the articulators and are more difficult to utter, requiring rather highly organized and co-ordinated movements17. although a variety of explanations have been offered for the findings as regards the loci of dysfluency, an interpretation that seems to be highly relevant in terms of this study, is one presented by wingate26. in accounting for the fact that word length and position of the word in the sentence are important factors in precipitating dysfluency, wingate has postulated a journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, deceber 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s o m e aspects of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic subjects 61 physiological explanation that might provide some support for the hypothesis that stuttering is a form of apraxia. he indicates that it is the initial sounds that are the focal points of attack and are the points at which the transition from inactivity to "energizing of motor functions" occurs. further, luria's15 description of an apraxic disturbance is relevant here. he considers that there is a loss in the selectivity of the innervation of the articulatery movements, manifesting itself in an inability, on the part of the patient, to assume the correct position of tongue and lips in the production of speech. wingate26 has also pointed out that the conspicuousness of the word in the speech sequence is not the most important factor, but that the longer the word, the more intricate the pattern of motor co-ordination required. in addition, the more intricate and complex the movements required of the patient, the more demanding the act of "motor planning" will be and the more inaccessible is the motor schema for that word. these interpretations could well be incorporated in the concepts of ideokinetic and ideational apraxias5·1 8. the factors of word position, word length and phonetic characteristics do . seem to play an important part in precipitating dysfluency in both stuttering and dysphasia, while the grammatical factor revealed what is generally noted in the primary stage of stuttering. 2. propositional aspects and the adaptation effect were the phenomena investigated in determining whether the dysfluencies of dysphasics are susceptible to influences that bring about modifications in stuttering behaviour. average severity rating frequency of dysfluency. % rate of utterance in words per min. f r e e prop. f r e e prop. f r e e prop. s i . 7,33 7,33 '34 26 35,39 25,73 s 2 . 2,33 2,33 16 38 118,5 37,73 s3. 3 3,66 27,33 37,14 72,49 139,07 s4. 2,33 2,33 14 14 88,41 43,86 s5. 6 5 38,67 64 66,37 29,88 table ii. comparison of severity and frequency, and rate of utterance on a 150-word sample of the free speech of dysphasic subjects and the propositional speech task. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging· vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde. vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 2 lesley caplan propositionality. three measures were used to determine the effect of propositionality on dysphasic dysfluency. on an equal-appearing interval scale, ratings of the severity of dysfluency showed no change from the free speech sample to the propositional task, while the frequency count also indicated no real change. however, the assessment of the rate of utterance, revealed a reasonably marked reduction in the rate of verbal output under conditions of propositionality (table 2). the measures used did not reveal a trend in the results, although there is evidence of consistency with individual subjects. despite the inconclusiveness of these results, it does seem that with more rigorous experimental procedures, there could be an increase in the severity and frequency of dysfluency when subjects encounter a situation in which their ability to propositionalize is focussed upon. the adaptation effect. on the question-and-answer technique devised by schaef19 adaptation scores approximated those found in stutterers. there was a decrease in the frequency of dysfluency from one trial to the next. on the test requiring repetition of presented sentences from "arthur the young rat" most of the subjects manifested positively accelerated adaptation curves i.e. there was an increase in the frequency and severity of dysfluency on the three consecutive trials involved in the task (table 3). it seems that these results can be explained by considering the effects of fatigue on responses of dysphasic patients. the increased fatigue during the second task (three consecutive repetitions of a single sentence) appears to have been greater than that in the first task, because each response on the first task was separated from succeeding responses by a question from the examiner. the subjects were, therefore, given a rest period in which some recovery could have occurred. goldstein8 referred to the evidence of fatigue in brain-damaged patients and he pointed out that fatigue results in manifestations resembling catastrophic responses, and that perseveration is considered by some authorities to be a catastrophic response or an attempt to avoid catastrophe. it seems then, that in dysphasics, repetition of a response rather than producing an inhibitary potential affiliated to fatigue2, results in unobstructed, persisting responses related to fatigue. the fact that fatigue is a behavioural response, characteristic of stressful situations8, and that the factor of stress played a major role in accounting for stuttering in terms of a breakdown theory where there is a failure in the neuromuscular organization of the individual, could account for the increase in the frequency of dysfluency on successive repetitions of the same material. this finding does seem to reject the hypothesis that a reduction of dysfluency will manifest itself in the speech of dysphasics during repeated readings of journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, deceber 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s o m e aspects of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic subjects. 6 3 νο οο on (ν ο ο ο νο 00 ττ ο ο ο ο ρί (ν ιλ fo on rc/3 ο c/3 cvj ο «λ ο ο γ ο ο · νο ο ο rίγί οο" ττ ο ο ο ιλ <ν (ν fo rm (ν fo <ν γνο γon νο \ © <ν (ν \ © cvj ο ο νο ο γ*^ ττ ο vo ο ο o n (ν «λ (ν (ν 'ίwm <ν ιλ ιλ on «λ cs cs ο ο <ν ο ττ ττ ο ο ο ο (ν «λ ί'ίο i 3 ή < o q tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 4 lesley caplan the same material. however, it appears that methods of eliciting adaptation do tend to bias results. possibly with further experimentation, and with refinements in techniques, the adaptation effect may be revealed to be a potential "discriminating tool" in the investigation of non-fluencies, irrespective of the origin16. conclusion as noted above the results of this study appear inconclusive although some trends can be identified. it cannot be said that the dysfluencies of dysphasia are alike or unlike those of stuttering, nor can anything be said as regards the behaviour of these dysfluencies under certain conditions. the nature and amount of the dysfluency of dysphasia were examined, and although the amount of dysfluency appeared to justify the label of stuttering, the nature of these dysfluencies differs from those observed in stuttering. despite this, trained judges were inclined to evaluate the speech as stuttered speech and it seems, therefore, that other more subtle attributes of the dysfluencies must be considered as contributing to the diagnosis of stuttering. possibly anxiety and tension as well as the retardation in the spontaneity of expression, are the operative factors. the loci of the dysfluencies in the sequence of dysphasic speech were investigated. in terms of the grammatical function of words, the majority of subjects experienced more difficulty on the function words of language, rather than on the content or lexical words which precipitate dysfluency in stutterers. all subjects experienced the greatest difficulty on words in the initial position in the sentence as is found in stutterers. for most of the subjects dysfluency was most frequently precipitated by longer words and consonants generally created more difficulty than vowels.· in investigating the phenomena that modify stuttering behaviour, propositionality and the adaptation effect were considered. under conditions of propositionality some increase in the frequency and severity of the dysfluencies of dysphasics was observed, while on the adaptation task divergent results were obtained. the study has, however, yielded a number of diagnostic, therapeautic and re-' search implications. possibly one of the most important, and in response to st. onge's20 plea for the division of stuttering symptoms into separate syndrome complexes, is the need for differential diagnosis in relation to the problem of stuttering. by knowing what type of dysfluencies predominate and whether the dysfluencies are susceptible to the adaptation effect, and in what way, the clinician may well be provided with a further clue in her attempts to make a differential diagnosis and plan appropriate therapy. in conjunction with other measures, these factors may prove instrumental in determining whether a child's dysfluencies are normal, the result of stressful environmental pressures in an organically normal individual, the overt manifestations of a word-finding difficulty, or symptoms of an inadequate neurophysiological organisation. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, deceber 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s o m e aspects of stuttering-like speech in adult dysphasic subjects 65 in the treatment of any disorder of speech or language where dysfluencies manifest themselves it may be important, therapeutically to be aware of the factors that are instrumental in determining the occurrence of the dysfluencies and to desensitize the patient to the influences of these factors. propositionality, it seems, can be considered to be responsible for much of the inconsistency and variability observed in dysphasic and stuttering responses, and the need to heighten the patient's level of tolerance to aspects of this phenomenon and to desensitize him to those factors that precipitate difficulty is important, difficulty is important. in conclusion this study could possibly be most profitably viewed as a preliminary in the investigation of the stuttering-like symptoms in expressive disorders of any type. examination of the hesitancies of normal speech, dysphasic and stuttered speech may well provide some data to add to the understanding of the process of language formulation and how it breaks down. further study along these lines may provide added information as to the nature of stuttering. references 1. arend, r., handzel, l. and weiss, b. (1962): dysphasic stuttering. folia phoniatrica, 14; 55-66. 2. bloodstein, 0 . (1969): a handbook on stuttering. national easter seal society for crippled children and adults, chicago, illinois. 3. brain, r. (1961): speech disorders. butterworths, london. 4. brown, s.f. (1945): the loci of stuttering in the speech sequence. j. speech dis., 10; 181-192. 5. canter, g.j. (1969): the influence of primary and secondary verbal apraxia on output disturbances and aphasic syndromes. paper presented to the annual meeting of asha, chicago, illinois. 6. critchley, m. (1970). aphasiology and other aspects of language. edward arnold (pub) ltd., london. 7. eisenson, j. (1958): a perseverance theory of stuttering. in: stuttering: a symposium. harper and row publishers, new york. 8. goldstein, k. (1948): language and language disturbances. grune and stratton, new york. 9. head, h. (1915): hughlings jackson on aphasia and kindred affections of speech. brain, 38; 1-27. 10. johnson, w. (1961): measurements of oral reading and speaking rate and dysfluency of adult male and female stutterers and nonstutterers./. speech hearing dis., monograph supplement no. 7, june; 1-20. 11. johnson, w„ darley, f.l. and spiestersbach, d.c. (1963): diagnostic methods in speech pathology, harper and row, new york. 12. karlin, i.w., (1947): a psychosomatic theory of stuttering./. speech dis., 12; 319-322. 13. lanham, l.w., (1968): lecture notes in phonetics and linguists ii, university of the witwatersrand. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 6 lesley caplan 14. lewis, d. and sherman, d; (1951): measuring the severity of stuttering. j. speech and hearing dis., 16; 320-326. 15. luria, a.r. (1966): higher cortical functions in man. tavistock publications, london. 16. newman, p.w. (1954): a study of adaptation and recovery of the stuttering response in self-formulated speech./. speech hearing dis., 19; 450-458. 17. quarrington, b., conway, j. and siegel, n. (1962): an experimental study of some properties of stuttered words./. speech hearing res., 5; 387-394. 18. reef, h. (1967): apraxic dysarthria./. s.a. logo. soc., 14; 37-44. 19. schaef, r.a. (1955): the use of questions to elicit stuttering adaptation./. speech hearing dis., 20; 262-265. 20. st. onge, k.r. (1963): the stuttering syndrome./. speech hearing res., 6; 195-197. 21. travis, l.e. (1931): speech pathology. d. appleton and co., new york and london. 22. van riper, c. (1971): the nature of stuttering. prentice-hall inc., englewood cliffs, new jersey. 23. weisenburg, t.h. and mcbride, e. (1935): aphasia: a clinical and psychological study. the commonwealth fund, new york. 24. weiss, d.a. (1967): similarities and differences between cluttering and stuttering. folia phoniatrica, 19; 98-104. 25. west, r. (1958). an agnostic's speculations about stuttering. in: stuttering: a symposium. harper and row publishers, new york. 26. wingate, h.e. (1967): stuttering and word length. /. speech hearing res., 10; 146-152. 27. young, μ .a. and prather, e.m. (1962): measuring the severity of stuttering using short segments of speech. /. speech hearing res., 5; 256-262. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, deceber 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y a r l editorial ύ^ηεν writing about speech therapy, as about any therapy, both the etiological and therapeutic aspects of speech disorders must be taken into account. we are pleased to print in this issue of the journal, articles which lay different stresses on these various aspects of speech and hearing defects. miss aron, who has recently returned from the united states and canada, stresses the therapeutic aspect of stuttering in her article, with particular reference to certain aspects of psychotherapy implicit in stuttering therapy. the relationship between stuttering therapy and psychotherapy, in this case, learning psychotherapy,, is indicated by miss marks in her article, and she also discusses the theoretical implications of considering stuttering as one aspect of the learning situation. one of the leading educationalists in this country is dr. behr,. .who has specialised in the problems of the deaf and hard-of-hearing child in this country and over-seas. we welcome his contribution "understanding the hard-of-hearing child," as being of great interest and value to speech therapists, and to teachers and parents of the aurally handicapped child. the two articles which describe case histories (in the case of miss whiting—diagnosis, and in the case of mrs. lubinsky, therapy) indicate the advance of practical work done in speech therapy in south africa, and further contributions along these lines would be welcomed for the journal. speech therapists in private practice miss m. marks, b.a. log. (rand) 5, jackal street, kensington. phone 24-2376. mrs. f. t. lubinsky. b.a. log. (rand) 26, caledon street, emmarentia extension, phone 46-3222. mrs. hilary meyersohn. b.a. log. (rand) 1 myer street, parkdene, boksburg. phone 52-2046. mies g. davidson, b.a. log. (rand) 1106, marble arch, goldreich street, hillbrow. johannesburg. phone 43-0463. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 99 spraakdiskriminasie by bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers liza van wyk, isabel uys en maggi soer departement kommunikasiepatologie, universiteit van pretoria opsomming in hierdie studie is die invloed van veroudering op die spraakdiskriminasievermoe van bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers ondersoek. 'n opname-metode is gebruik en dertig proefpersone, met dieselfde graad van perifere gehoorafname, is in twee groepe van vyftien persone elk verdeel. groep 1 het bestaan uit bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers wat geen of geringe spraakdiskriminasieprobleme ervaar, terwyl groep 2 ernstige spraakdiskriminasieprobleme ervaar. 'n verskeidenheid toetse, naamlik die sintetiese sinsidentifikasietoets, die verspringende spondeewoordtoets en die aanbieding van monosillabiese woorde tesame met 'n ipsi-kompeterende spraakboodskap, is gebruik om hul spraakdiskriminasievermoe in verskillende luistersituasies te bepaal. 'n selfevaluasieskaal is ook deur die bejaardes voltooi. die resultate van hierdie studie toon dat die verouderingsproses en die effek hiervan op veral sentrale ouditiewe prosessering, bydra tot die kompleksiteit en uiteenlopendheid van bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers se spraakdiskriminasieprobleme. die resultate beklemtoon verder die noodsaaklikheid van volledige intervensieprogramme vir bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers. abstract this study investigates the influence of aging on the speech discrimination abilities of elderly hearing instrument users. a survey method was used and thirty subjects, with the same degree of peripheral hearing loss, were divided into two groups of fifteen persons each. group 1 consisted of elderly hearing instrument users who experienced little or no speech discrimination problems while group 2 consisted of persons with severe speech discrimination problems. the synthetic sentence identification test, the staggered spondaic word test and phonemically balanced words with an ipsi-competitive speech noise, were used to ascertain their speech discrimination capabilities in different listening situations. the elderly also completed a self-evaluation scale. the results of the study indicate that the aging process and changes in central auditory processing contribute to the complexity and diversity of elderly hearing instrument user's speech discrimination problems. the results also stress the importance of comprehensive intervention programmes for elderly hearing instrument users. sleutelwoorde: spraakdiskriminasie, bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers, sentrale ouditiewe prosessering. die verouderingsproses blyk die dominante oorsaak van gehoorprobleme by bejaardes te wees (working group on speech understanding and aging, 1988). histopatologiese en morfologiese studies bevestig veranderinge in die totale gehoorsisteem wat die perifere gehoormeganisme, die koglea, gehoorsenuwee, breinstam en die temporale lobbe insluit (willott, 1991). hierdie ouderdomsverwante veranderinge in die ouditiewe sisteem kan ouditief presenteer as 'n perifere gehoorverlies, 'n sentrale ouditiewe prosesseringsprobleem of as 'n kombinasie van die twee verskynsels. 'n verskeidenheid ander faktore kan ook gekoppel word aan die gehooren spraakdiskriminasieprobleme wat bejaardes ervaar. metaboliese veranderinge, vaskulere abnormaliteite, nierprobleme, medikasie, bepaalde mediese behandeling en geraasblootstelling kan ook die gehoor van die bejaarde bei'nvloed (willott, 1991). bepaaldejfaktore soos neuro-anatomiese veranderinge as gevolg van die verouderingsproses, degenerasie van bepaalde kognitiewe funksies soos geheue, aandag, spoed van prosessering en ook linguistiesouditiewe funksies blyk 'n negatiewe invloed op die totale ouditiewe prosesseringsvermoe van die bejaarde te he (lemme & hedberg, 1988; marshall, 1981; cohen, 1987 & willott, 1991). die passing van gehoorapparate word beskou as 'n primere rehabilitasiestrategie om gehoorgestremde bejaardes se gehoor te verbeter (hull, 1985). 'n groot persentasie van die bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers ervaar egter steeds probleme met kommunikasie en veral met spraakdiskriminasie in die teenwoordigheid van agtergrondgeraas, ten spyte van klankversterking (stach, 1994). hierdie probleme presenteer egter nie in dieselfde mate by gehoorapparaatgebruikers met dieselfde graad van perifere gehoorafname nie (stach, 1994; hull, 1985). spraakdiskriminasieprobleme is in die verlede veral toegeskryf aan die afname in perifere gehoorsensitiwiteit die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 100 liza van wyk, isabel uys & maggi soer (jerger, jerger, oliver & pirozzolo, 1989; humes & roberts, 1990). resente navorsing dui egter daarop dat die ouderdomsverwante veranderinge in die sentrale ouditiewe sisteem ook tot spraakdiskriminasieprobleme kanbydra (rodriguez, disarno & hardiman, 1990; jerger, et al., 1989; marshall, 1981). 'n kombinasie van die effek van 'n perifere gehoorverlies en die afname in sentrale ouditiewe prosessering is nie uitgesluit nie (willott, 1991). daar is dus geen uitsluitsel oor die ware aard en oorsake van spraakdiskriminasieprobleme soos wat dit presenteer by bejaardes nie, maar die betrokkenheid van die sentrale ouditiewe sisteem geniet toenemende aandag in die jongste navorsingsliteratuur (rodriguez, et al., 1990; jerger, et al., 1989). klem word tans ook veral geplaas op die gebruik van sentrale spraaktoetse om sentrale ouditiewe funksionering te evalueer (stach, 1994). die motivering vir hierdie studie word gerig deur bepaalde kontroversies, leemtes en 'n behoefte aan kennis oor gehoorprobleme en veral spraakdiskriminasieprobleme soos wat dit presenteer by die bejaarde en veral by die bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruiker. vanwee die kompleksiteit van die bejaarde se ouditiewe probleme bestaan daar ook leemtes ten opsigte van die toetsing, diagnose en rehabilitasie van die bejaarde met gehoorprobleme. hierdie leemtes en 'n behoefte aan meer kennis van die bejaarde se unieke ouditiewe probleme moet dus aangespreek word. metode doel die hoofdoelstellings van hierdie studie is om die invloed van veroudering op die spraakdiskriminasievermoe van bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers, met dieselfde graad van perifere gehoorverlies, te ondersoek en om te bepaal waarom sommige bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers meer probleme met spraakdiskriminasie ondervind as ander (van wyk, 1997). die hoofdoelstellings word gerealiseer in die volgende subdoelstellings: 1. om spraakdiskriminasie in stilte van twee groepe bejaardes, naamlik bejaardes met geringe spraakdiskriminasieprobleme en bejaardes met ernstige spraakdiskriminasieprobleme, te bepaal en te vergelyk. 2. om die spraakdiskriminasievermoe van die twee genoemde groepe in komplekse luistersituasies te bepaal en te vergelyk. 3. om die twee groepe bejaardes se subjektiewe ervaring van hul gehoorprobleme en die baat wat hul vind by die dra van gehoorapparate te bepaal en te vergelyk. 4. om te bepaal of daar 'n verband is tussen die twee groepe se subjektiewe evaluasie van hul gehoorprobleme (sonder gehoorapparate) en die objektiewe oudiologiese evaluering van hul perifere gehoorstatus. 5. om te bepaal of daar 'n korrelasie is tussen die bejaardes se subjektiewe evaluasie van hul gehoorfunksionering en die objektiewe meting van hul spraakdiskriminasievermoe in verskillende luistersituasies. navorsings ontwerp die bepaalde navorsingstrategie wat in hierdie studie gebruik is, is die opname-metode (jacobs, haasbroek & theron, 1992). die kwalitatiewe-kwantitatiewe aard van hierdie navorsing verseker die realisering van die doelstellings (jacobs, et al., 1992). proefpersone dertig bejaardes is op 'n afhanklike, ewekansige wyse geselekteer en in die twee navorsingsgroepe verdeel (jacobs, et al., 1992). die proefpersone het aan die volgende kriteria voldoen: 1. afrikaans moet die moedertaal wees, aangesien alle toetsstimuli in afrikaans aangebied is. navorsing toon dat sentrale toetse wat nie in die klient se moedertaal aangebied word nie, aanleiding kan gee tot swakker resultate wat nie net verklaarbaar is op grond van sentrale ouditiewe funksionering nie (lemme & hedberg, 1988). 2. persone vanaf 65 jaar word vir die doeleindes van hierdie studie beskou as bejaard en is dus ingesluit. ouderdomme wissel tussen 65-90 jaar. deur die insluiting van bejaardes oor die totale ouderdomspektrum kan bepaal word of daar 'n verband is tussen veroudering en die mate waarin spraakdiskriminasieprobleme voorkom (willott, 1991). 3. sowel mans as vrouens is geselekteer, om te verseker dat spraakdiskriminasie by die bejaarde populasie, soos wat hulle in die samelewing leef, ondersoek word (marshall, 1981). 4. normale middeloorfunksionering is 'n vereiste om sodoende te verseker dat dit nie 'n invloed op die perifere gehoorstatus van die bejaardes het nie (margolis & shanks, 1985). 5. bejaardes met binourale, geringe tot gemiddelde, sensories-neurale hoefrekwensie-gehoorverliese is geselekteer. hierdie oudiometriese konfigurasie is kenmerkend van presbikusie (willott, 1991). dit impliseer gehoorverliese waarvan die gemiddelde suiwertoondrempels (by 500 hz, 1000hz en 2000hz) >25db en <56db is (yantis, 1994). vir hierdie studie is bejaardes met gemiddelde suiwertoondrempels van 40db-50db geselekteer om eenvormigheid te verseker. die hoer frekwensies is ook in ag geneem, aarigesien hierdie gehoordrempels 'n belangrike invloed op spraakdiskriminasie het. drempels tussen ongeveer 50db-70db by 4000hz en 60db-80db by 8000hz is aanvaar en dit pas in by die beskrywing van presbikusie (willott, 1991). i 6. bejaardes se mediese geskiedenis moet vry wees van enige serebro-vaskulere insidente, seniliteit enjenige geslotehoofbeserings of enige ander siektetoestand, aangesien dit moontlik 'n negatiewe invloed op die toetsresultate kan he (willott, 1991). die bejaardes mag ook geen ototoksiese middels of enige ander medikasie gebruik wat 'n remmende invloed op die werking van die sentrale senuweesisteem kan he nie (rodriguez et al., 1990; jerger et al., 1989; marshall, 1981). ' 7. alle proefpersone is aan 'n siftingsprosedure onderwerp, om te bepaal of kognitiewe en ook linguistiese vaardighede intakt is, aangesien hierdie faktore ook die spraakdiskriminasievermoe kan bei'nvloed. (cohen, 1987; lemme & hedberg, 1988). / die volgende materiaal en apparaat is gebruik om die dertig bejaardes vir hierdie studie te selekteer (van wyk, 1997): the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spraakdiskriminasie by bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers 101 'n gevalgeskiedenisvorm is gebruik, om identifiserende inligting en ook ander inligting soos die mediese geskiedenis, aanvang van gehoorverlies en kommunikasieprobleme, op te teken (rosenberg, 1978); die mini mayo mental scale is gebruik om kognitiewe funksionering te evalueer (folstein, folstein & mchugh, 1975); . subtoetse van die bostonse diagnostiese afasietoets is gebruik om linguistiese vermoe te evalueer (goodglass & kaplan, 1983); . alle oudiometriese toetsing is met 'n gsi 16-oudiometer uitgevoer en voldoen aan die vereiste van die sabs 0154-1983 (suid-afrikaanse buro vir standaarde, 1994). die oudiometer is geyk op 1995-01-04. tdh-50poorfone, gemonteer in mx-51-kussings is vir suiwertoonoudiometrie gebruik. 'n b-17-beengeleier is vir beengeleidingstoetse gebruik. toetse is in 'n klankdigte kamer van industrial acoustics company inc. uitgevoer en die toetsomgewing voldoen aan die sabs 0182-1982standaard (suid-afrikaanse buro vir standaarde, 1994). 'n gsi-33-middelooranaliseerder is gebruik om immittansietoetsing uit te voer. 'n heine alpha nica tron 2-otoskoop is gebruik om die otoskopiese ondersoek op die proefpersone uit te voer. bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers, wat die phonakgehoorsentrum (pretoria) tussen januarie 1993 en januarie 1995 besoek het, is tydens die seleksie van proefpersone genader. bejaardes wat hul gehoorapparate daagliks vir ongeveer twee jaar gedra het, is geselekteer vir die studie. sodoende is verseker dat bejaardes reeds aangepas het by die dra van gehoorapparate en is eenvormigheid ten opsigte van klankversterking verseker. op grond van die bejaardes se suiwertoondrempels en subjektiewe evaluasie van hul gehoorprobleme is dertig bejaardes op 'n afhanklike, ewekansige wyse vanuit 'n totale populasie van 485 bejaardes geselekteer en in twee groepe verdeel, naamlik bejaardes met geen of geringe spraakdiskriminasieprobleme en bejaardes met ernstige spraakdiskriminasieprobleme. sowel groep 1 as groep 2 het bestaan uit sewe vrouens en agt mans. die dertig persone is telefonies gekontak waartydens 'n afspraak vir 'n konsultasie gemaak is. | tydens 'n konsultasie is 'n gestruktureerde onderhoud gebruik om 'n volledige gevalgeskiedenis van die bejaardes te verkry en die kognitiewe en linguistiese vaardighede is ook geevalueer. 'n otoskopiese ondersoek is op elke proefpersoon voor die oudiologiese toetsing uitgevoer. 'n timpanogram is van elke proefpersoon verkry en elke proefpersoon moes aan die gestelde norme voldoen soos bespreek in die literatuur om as proefpersoon geselekteer te word (margolis & shanks, 1985). 'n volledige suiwertoonoudiogram is verkry, spraakontvangsdrempelbepaling en spraakdiskriminasietoetsing in stilte is ook uitgevoer (van wyk, 1997). die uitvoering van hierdie prosedures stem ooreen soos bespreek in bestaande literatuur (van wyk, 1997; yantis, 1994; penrod, 1994). tabel 1 verskaf 'n opsomming van die eienskappe van die dertig proefpersone wat vir hierdie studie gebruik is (van wyk, 1997). materiaal en ap par a at die materiaal en apparaat wat in hierdie studie gebruik is, word in twee onderafdelings bespreek, naamlik: a. materiaal en apparaat gebruik vir die insameling van data die volgende apparaat is tydens die insameling van data gebruik (van wyk, 1997): die gsi 16-oudiometer (geyk op 1995-01-04) is ook gebruik tydens die uitvoering van spesiale spraaktoetse. alle toetse is in dieselfde klankdigte kamer uitgevoer soos reeds voorheen gespesifiseer. 'n tweekanaalbandopnemer, naamlik 'n marantz stereo cassette deck sd-255, is aan die gespesifiseerde oudiometer gekoppel vir die uitvoer van die spraaktoetse. 'n hoekwaliteit tdk d90-kasset is gebruik vir die opneem van die kompeterende boodskap, die onsinsinne en ook die digotiese toetsprosedure, wat gebruik is tydens die uitvoering van die komplekse spraaktoetse. tabel 2 verskaf 'n oorsig van en motivering vir die verskillende toetse en materiaal wat tydens die studie gebruik is: die siftings-, die basiese en ook die spesiale sentrale-toetse, asook die selfevaluasieskaal. b. materiaal vir die optekening en verwerking van data response van die proefpersone in die verskillende toetse, is op bestaande responsvorms, wat in die kliniese opset gebruik word, opgeteken (van wyk, 1997). statistiese verwerkings is gedoen met behulp van 'n ibm 370rekenaar en die statistical packages for social sciences program, universiteit van pretoria (theron, 1995). dataversamelingsprosed ures die volgende dataversamelingsprosedures is in hierdie studie gevolg en word in twee onderafdelings bespreek, naamlik: a. dataversamelingsprosedures wat uitgevoer is tydens die navorsing alle spraaktoetse en spesiale toetse is uitgevoer deur gebruik te maak van bandopnames. sodoende is verseker dat die aanbieding van toetse op 'n konstante wyse plaasgevind het en is die toetsgeldigheid en -betroubaarheid verhoog (penrod, 1994). spraakdiskriminasietoetsing in stilte is uitgevoer by 50db, 40db, 30db, 20db en lodb bokant die spraakontvangsdrempel. by 50db bokant die spraakontvangsdrempel is bepaal of 'n "rollover" voorkom (penrod, 1994). die sintetiese sinsidentifikasietoets is by dieselfde aanbiedingsintensiteit as bogenoemde uitgevoer ten einde die verskillende resultate met mekaar te vergelyk (kaplan, et al., 1984; van wyk, 1997; willeford, 1985). die verspringende spondeewoordtoets is so aangebied dat die proefpersone die spondeewoorde in die regsnie-kompeterende (r-n-k); regs-kompeterende (r-k); links-kompeterende (l-k) en links-nie-kompeterende (l-n-k) luistersituasies moes herhaal (kaplan, et al., 1984). die toets is by 40db bokant die gemiddelde suiwertoondrempels by 500hz, 1000hz en 2000hz aangebied (lukas & genchur-lukas, 1985). die monosillabiese woorde tesame met die ipsidie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 102 liza van wyk, isabel uys & maggi soer m ak si m u m ko rr ek te sp ra ak di sk ri m ina si e re gt er oo r 10 0 % 10 0 % 10 0% 96 % 10 0 % 10 0% 10 0% 96 % 10 0% 10 0 % 96 % 10 0% 10 0 % 88 % 10 0% 98 ,4 % < n < n o o < n c d < n o < n c o o 2 c o o o c o < n o o i c o o o o c o o o c o ^ o o o o o j | 91 ,5 % m ak si m u m ko rr ek te sp ra ak di sk ri m ina si e lin ke roo r ο ο ο , η ο ο ο ο ο ο ^ , ο ο ^ , ο o o o g o o o o o o g o o g j o 98 ,9 % o i a i c o o o o c o o o c o o o c o ^ o j 91 ,5 % so d ( d b ) re gt er oo r c q c q c q c q c q c q c q c q p q c q c q c q c q c q c q ό ό ' ό ό ό ' ό ό ' ό ό ό ό ό ό ' ό ό ο ο ΐ β ί ο ο ί ο ο ο ί ο ΐ ο ΐ λ ο ο ί ο ο 43 ,7 db fflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflffl ό ό ό ό *"0 *"0 ό *"0 *"0 ό ό *ό *ό *ό *ό ο ο ι η ο ο ο ο ο ι ο ι ο ο ο ο ι ο ι ο 46 ,3 d b so d ( db ) lin ke roo r cqcqcqcqcqcqcqcqcq cq ffl ffl cq ffl ffl ό ό "ό ό "ό ό ό ό ό ι ο ι λ ι λ ο ο ο ο ο ι λ ι ο ι ο ι λ ο ι λ ι ο 45 ,3 db mfflfflfflfflfflwfflfflcqfflfflcqcqcq ό ό ό ό ό "ό ό "ό "ό ό "ό ό *"0 o o l o i f l o w w w o m o o o w l f l 46 ,7 d b st d ( db ) re gt er oo r fqcqfqcqpqcqpqcqpqcqpqp^pqp^p^ ' " ο ' ό ' ό ό ό ' ό ό ' ό ό ' ό ' ό ό ό ό ό o p i c o l o c o o o o w o n o n o n o 42 ,2 db pq pq pq pq pq pq pq" pq pq pq pq pq pq pq pq ό ό *"0 ό *"0 *"0 *"0 ό *"0 ό *ό "ό "ό *"0 *"0 ( n c o i o o o o o o o o c o t > c o o o o o o c o 45 ,5 db st d (d b ) lin ke roo r mfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflfflffl ό ό ό ' ό ' ό ό ' ό ' ό ' ό ό ό ό ' ό ό ό o l o l o o t > t > o o c o ( n c o o c o t > c o o 42 ,2 d b p q p q p q p q p q p q p q p q p q p q p q p q p q p q p q ό ό ό *"0 ό ό *"0 ό ό ό *"0 " ό " ό *"0 0 ( n l o l o c o c o t > < n t > c o o c o t > 0 ( n ι ο ι ο t f t f t f t f t f t f t f t f t f t f t f t f t f 45 ,6 d b im m it ta ns ie m et in gs re su lt at e re gt er oo r < < < < < < < < < < < < < < < a j a j a j o q j o j q j a j o a j a j q j o a j q j h h h h h h h h h h h h h h h ο ο ο ο < < < < < < < < < < < < < < < α ι α ι < υ < υ ( υ < υ < υ ω ω < υ < υ < υ < υ < υ < υ ρ , ο , ο . ο . ο . ο ο ο . ο ο ο ο ' 1 ' 1 ' 1 ρ ρ η ρ η η η η η η η ρ η η η •χ* -μ-::im m it ta ns ie m et in gs re su lt at e lin ke roo r ο ο < < < < < < < < < < < < < < < a j a j a j a j a j a j a j a j o a j a j a j a j a j q j h h h h h h h h h h h h h h p * * < < < < < < < < < < < < < < < c i u v a o i c o i u c o v u u o ) ^ o o o o o o o o o o o o g g g η η η η ρ η η ρ η η η ρ η η η -μ•'.· -χo to sk opi es e on de rso ek no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l no rm aa l l in gu is ti es e ve rm oe (t ot al e aa nt al pu nt e) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 18 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 18 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) 11 8, 9 in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 18 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 18 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) in ta kt (1 18 ) in ta kt (1 16 ) in ta kt (1 16 ) in ta kt (1 16 ) in ta kt (1 19 ) 11 8, 2 k og n iti ew e ve rm oe (t ot al e aa nt al pu nt e) in ta kt ( 37 ) in ta kt ( 36 ) in ta kt ( 37 ) in ta kt (3 8) in ta kt ( 37 ) in ta kt ( 38 ) in ta kt (3 8) in ta kt ( 36 ) in ta kt (3 7) in ta kt ( 37 ) in ta k t( 35 ) in ta kt ( 38 ) in ta kt (3 5) in ta kt ( 37 ) in ta kt ( 36 ) 36 ,8 in ta kt ( 36 ) in ta kt ( 37 ) in ta kt ( 35 ) in ta kt ( 38 ) in ta kt ( 36 ) in ta kt (3 8) in ta kt ( 35 ) in ta kt ( 32 ) in ta kt ( 33 ) in ta kt (3 8) in ta kt ( 37 ) in ta kt ( 34 ) in ta kt ( 32 ) in ta kt (3 2) in ta kt ( 37 ) 35 ,3 k ro n olo gi es e ou de rdo m 65 jr 0 m nd e 65 jr 2 m nd e 67 jr 1 1 m nd e 69 jr 1 1 m nd e 70 jr 7 m nd e 73 jr 9 m nd e 73 jr 1 0 m nd e 74 jr 6 m nd e 75 jr 1 m nd 76 jr 6 m nd e 77 j r 10 m nd e 80 jr 4 m nd e 81 jr 1 1 m nd e 82 j r 7 m nd e 84 jr 6 m nd e 74 jr 1 m n d e 65 j r 6 m nd e 67 jr 1 1 m nd e 69 jr 1 1 m nd e 70 jr 1 0 m nd e 72 j r 0 m nd e 73 j r 2 m nd e 74 jr 6 m nd e 75 jr 2 m nd e 76 jr 4 m nd e 77 jr 1 0 m nd e 78 jr 1 0 m nd e 82 jr 4 m nd e 83 j r 11 m nd e 84 jr 1 m nd 84 jr 4 m nd e 75 j r 3 m n d e g es la g y p ro ef pe rs oo nno m m er c c t o d o i o h i n w ' i l f i t o c o d o l o h h h h w n c 1 c j n w n n i n n c 1 g ro ep •η g e m . ( n g e m . c cu -g μ =3 >> c ο μ ιλ ω c 'α ce ω c -q txo d ο < the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) kdiskriminasie by bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers tabel 2: oorsig van die verskillende toetse toetse motivering vir keuse van toetse verwysing 1. siftingstoetse * mini mayo mental dien as siftingsinstrument om kognitiewe vaardighede soos orientasie, aandag, lees, leer, rekenkunde, abstraksie en algemene kennis te evalueer. folstein, et al., 1975 * bostonse diagnostiese afasietoets dien as siftingsinstrument om linguistiese vaardighede soos, woordvindingsvermoe, ouditiewe begrip en verbale uitdrukkings te evalueer. goodglass & kaplan, 1983 2. basiese toetsbattery * gevalgeskiedenis bevat identifiserende inligting van bejaarde, asook inligting soos, aanvang van gehoorverlies, simptome, oorsake, mediese geskiedenis en kommunikasievaardighede. rosenberg, 1978 * otoskopiese ondersoek evalueer status van eksterne oorgedeelte, oorkanaal en timpaniese membraan. yantis, 1994 * suiwertoonoudiogram (lugen beengeleiding) bepaal luggeleidingsdrempels by 125hz-8000hz en beengeleidingsdrempels by 250hz-4000hz ten einde die graad en aard van die perifere gehoorverlies te bepaal. yantis, 1994 * spraakontvangsdrempel (sod) ingesluit om te bepaal waar proefpersone 50% van spondeewoorde kan identifiseer. dien ook as basis waarvolgens die intensiteit vir aanbieding van ander toetsstimuli bepaal word en om std te kontroleer. penrod, 1994 * spraakdiskriminasie in stilte bepaal spraakdiskriminasievermoe in stilte om die spraakdiskriminasie kurwe te verkry waar bejaardes hul fg-maksimum en 50% korrekte spraakdiskriminasie behaal. penrod, 1994 laubscher & tesner, 1996 * akoestiese immittansiemeting 1 evalueer middeloorfunksionering om onder andere die statiese beweeglikheid en fisiese volume te bepaal margolis & shanks, 1985 1 3. spesiale sentr|ale-toetse * sintetiese sinsevalueer sentrale ouditiewe funksie en spraakdiskriminasievermoe. differensiale diagnostiese toets om te onderskei tussen kogleere, retro-kogleere en sentrale letsel. jerger, et al., 1989 kaplan, et al., 1984 willeford, 1985 muller, 1981 * verspringende spondeewoordtoets (vsw-toets) ^ evalueer spraakdiskriminasievermoe in onder andere, komplekse luistersituasies en kompenseer vir die perifere gehoorverlies. kaplan, et al., 1984 lukas & genchurlukas, 1985 * monosillabiese woorde en die aanbieding van 'n ipsi-konipeterende boodskap identifiseer spraakdiskriminasieprobleme tydens die aanbieding van 'n ipsi-kompeterende boodskap (sein-tot-ruis-verhouding : + 5 db). penrod, 1994 stach, 1994 4. self-evaluasie skaal * "hearing handicap inventory for the elderly" (hhie) subjektiewe inligting deur die bejaarde verskaf wat aanvullend is tot die objektiewe oudiometriese data; evalueer situasieen emosionele veranderlikes van 'n gehoorverlies wat 'n invloed kan he op alledaagse funksionering; verskaf inligting oor evaluasie sonder en met gehoorapparate. ventry & weinstein, 1982 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 104 liza van wyk, isabel uys & maggi soer kompeterende boodskap is aangebied 40db bokant die spraakontvangsdrempel. die sein-tot-ruis-verhouding was +5db (penrod, 1994). die selfevaluasieskaal is twee keer tydens 'n gestruktureerde onderhoud voltooi. tydens die eerste sessie het die proefpersone die skaal voltooi asof hulle nie gehoorapparate dra nie en die tweede keer is die skaal voltooi asof hulle gehoorapparate dra (ventry & weinstein, 1982). b. data-optekeningen dataverwerkingsprosedures response van die 30 proefpersone is opgeteken op die verskillende responsvorms, soos reeds bespreek. die dataop tekeningsprosedures, wat betref die resultate van die oudiometriese toetsing, stem ooreen met die optekeningsprosedures wat in die praktyk gebruik word. al die relevante toetsresultate is vir elkeen van die 30 proefpersone opgeteken op 'n individuele responsvorm. tydens die analise en prosessering van die data is daar gebruik gemaak van beskrywende en nie-parametriese statistiek (theron, 1995). data is verwerk met 'n ibm 370rekenaar en die statistical packages for social sciences program, universiteit van pretoria, (theron, 1995). in tabel 3 word 'n opsomming van die statistiese prosedures verskaf wat gebruik is om elke subdoelstelling te beantwoord (van wyk, 1997). resultate tabel 4 bied 'n opsomming van die twee navorsingsgroepe se gemiddelde resultate op die verskillende toetse (van wyk, 1997). dit blyk duidelik dat daar 'n beduidende verskil tussen die resultate van die twee groepe is, behalwe ten opsigte van die totale prestasie met en sonder gehoorapparate. 1. suiwertoonresultate van die bejaardes die gemiddelde suiwertoondrempels vir groep 1 is 42,2db vir beide ore. groep 2 se gemiddelde suiwertoondrempels is 45,6db vir die linkeren 45,5db vir die regterore. daar is dus nie 'n beduidende verskil tussen die resultate van groep 1 en groep 2 op die 5% vlak van beduidenheid nie. daar is ook nie 'n beduidende verskil tussen die regteren linkerore van elke groep afsonderlik nie (p < 0,05). 2. spraakdiskriminasievermoe in stilte die maksimum-gemiddelde prestasie by 40db bo die spraakontvangsdrempel vir groep 1 was 98,9% vir die linkerore en 98,4% vir die regterore. die maksimumgemiddelde prestasie by dieselfde intensiteitsvlak vir groep 2 was 91,5% vir beide ore. groep 2 presteer beduidend swakker as groep 1 by al die verskillende intensiteitsvlakke (p < 0,05). 'n tweede afleiding is dat daar geen beduidende verskil tussen die linkeren regterore van elke groep afsonderlik vir die toets bestaan nie (groep 1: ρ = 0,1056 en groep 2: ρ = 0,180). daar is verder geen toename in spraakdiskriminasieprobleme, soos ouderdom toeneem nie. daar is ook nie 'n beduidende verskil tussen die spraakdiskriminasieresultate van mans en vrouens nie (p = 0,2049). die "rollover-" fenomeen, in beide ore, is by drie bejaardes in groep 2 ge'identifiseer. 3. spraakdiskriminasievermoe in komplekse luistersituasies die resultate in die verskillende komplekse luistersituasies is soos volg: 3.1. prestasie in die sintetiese sinsidentifikasietoets groep 1 se maksimum-gemiddelde prestasie is 68,0% in die linkeren 66,9% in die regterore. groep 2 behaal 24,6% vir die linkeren 22,7% vir die regterore. groep 2 presteer dus beduidend swakker as groep 1 tydens die aanbieding en identifikasie van die sinne met 'n sein-tot-ruisverhouding van odb (p < 0,05). ander resultate dui daarop dat daar geen beduidende verskil tussen die prestasie van die linkeren regterore van elke groep se ssi-pi-funksie afsonderlik is nie (p-waarde vir groep 1 is 0,1056 en vir groep 2 is dit 0,2049). die gemiddelde ssi-pi-funksie is vir beide groepe beduidend swakker as die fg-pi-funksie, maar groep 2 (p = 0,000) se resultate wyk meer af as die van groep 1 (p = 0,0003). die "rollover-" fenomeen, in beide ore, het voorgekom by sewe bejaardes in groep 2. die tweede diagnostiese prosedure van die ssi-toets is om die ssi-ikb-resultate met die ssi-kkb-resultate te vergelyk. daar is 'n beduidende verskil tussen die ssiikb-resultate van die twee groepe, waar die sein-tot-ruisverhouding +10db, odb en -lodb is. die prestasie van groep 2 is beduidend swakker as die prestasie van groep 1 (p < 0,05). met 'n sein-tot-ruis-verhouding van -20db was daar egter geen beduidende verskil tussen die twee groepe nie. die ssi-kkb-resultate, met die sein-tot-ruisverhoudings odb, -20db en -40db, dui daarop dat groep 2 beduidend swakker gevaar het as groep 1. daar is egter geen beduidende verskil tussen die linkeren regterore van elke groep se resultate afsonderlik nie (p = 0,1474 vir groep 1 en vir groep 2 is ρ = 0,2241). ssi-ikb-afwykings is by albei groepe groter as ssi-kkb afwykings. ander resultate is dat daar geen beduidende verskil tussen mans en dames se prestasie is nie (p = 0,7938). 3.2. toetsresultate van die verspringende spondeewoordtoets ten opsigte van die totale prestasie-korrekte diskriminasie, behaal groep 1, 56,7% in die linkeren 82,7% in die regterore. groep 2 behaal 36,1% in die linkeren 58,3% in die regterore. na aanleiding van die 5%-vlak (p < 0,05) van beduidenheid is daar 'n beduidende verskil tussen die twee groepe se prestasie in die vier verskillende luisteromstandighede (r-n-k; r-k; l-k en l-n-k).' dit geld vir sowel die rou-vswen "corrected"-vsw-resultate. resultate toon verder dat daar nie 'n beduidende verskil tussen die r-vswen c-vsw-resultate van groep 1 is nie (p < 0,05). vir groep 2 is daar egter 'n beduidende verskil tussen die r-vswen c-vsw-resultate by al vier die luisteromstandighede (vir r-n-k is ρ = 0,0004; vir r7k is ρ = 0,0004; vir l-k en l-n-k is ρ = 0,0007). dit blyk dus dat groep 2 beduidend meer probleme as groep 1 in beide linkeren regteroor kompeterende situasies ondervind. / 3.3. prestasie tydens die aanbieding van monosillabiese woorde en 'n ipsi-kompeter'ende boodskap die maksimum-gemiddelde prestasie vir groep 1 se linkerore is 63,7% en vir die regterore 66,7%. die maksimum-gemiddelde prestasie vir groep 2 is 48,3% vir the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) spraakdiskriminasie by bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers 105 tabel 3: 'n opsomming van die verskillende statistiese prosedures wat gebruik is om elke subdoelstelling te beantwoord subdoelstellings prosedures beskrywing van statistiese verwysings 1. om die spraakdiskriminasievermoe van die twee navorsingsgroepe in stilte te bepaal en te vergelyk. 1.1 die algemene liniere metode is gebruik om die rekenkundige gemiddelde vir elke groep vir die verskillende toetsresultate te bepaal; 1.2 die mann whitney u-wilcoxon rank sum wtoets (5% vlak van beduidenheid) is gebruik om te bepaal of daar 'n beduidende verskil tussen die resultate van die twee groepe is. theron, 1995; jacobs, et al., 1992 2. om die spraakdiskriminasievermoe van die twee groepe in komplekse situasies te bepaal en te vergelyk. 2.1 die algemene liniere metode is gebruik om die rekenkundige gemiddelde prestasie van elke groep in die verskillende toetse te bepaal; 2.2 die mann whitney u-wilcoxon rank sum w-toets (5% vlak van beduidenheid) is gebruik om te bepaal of daar 'n beduidende verskil tussen die resultate van die twee groepe is; 2.3 die kendall en spearman-toetse is gebruik om te bepaal of daar 'n beduidende korrelasie tussen die resultate van die verskillende spesiale spraaktoetse is. theron, 1995. theron, 1995; jacobs, et al., 1992. theron, 1995; jacobs, et al., 1992. 3. om die twee groepe se subjektiewe ervaring van hul gehoorprobleme en die baat wat hulle vind by die dra van gehoorapparaat te bepaal en te vergelyk. 3.1 die algemene liniere metode is gebruik om die rekeningkundige gemiddelde vir die prestasie met en sonder gehoorapparate te bepaal; 3.2 die mann whitney u-wilcoxon rank sum w-toets (5% vlak van beduidenheid) is gebruik om te bepaal of daar 'n beduidende verskil tussen die resultate van die twee groepe is. theron, 1995; jacobs, et al., 1992 theron, 1995; jacobs, et al., 1992 4. om te bepaal of daar 'n korrelasie is tussen die bejaardes se subjektiewe evaluasie van hul gehoorprobleme en die objektiewe oudiologiese evaluering van hul perifere gehoorstatus. j 4.1 die kendall en spearman toetse is gebruik om te bepaal of daar 'n beduidende korrelasie tussen die resultate van die verskillende spesiale spraaktoetse is. theron, 1995; jacobs, et al., 1992 5. om te bepaal of daar 'n korrelasie is tussen die bejaardesjse subjektiewe evaluering van hul gehoorfunksionering en die objektiewe meting van hulle spraakdiskriminasievermoe. ] 5.1 die kendall en spearman toetse is gebruik om te bepaal of daar 'n beduidende korrelasie tussen die resultate van die verskillende spesiale spraaktoetse is. theron, 1995; jacobs, et al., 1992 tabel 4: opsomming van die verskillende toetsresultate gemiddelde maksimum prestasie, (%) fg-maksimum l r ssi-maksimum l r c-vsw l r monosillabiese woorde en 'n ipsi-kompeterende boodskap l r hhie(totaal) sonder met gehoorapparate groep 1 98,9 98,4 68,0 66,0 56,7 82,7 63,7 66,7 56,3 76,8 groep 2 91,5 91,5 27,7 22,6 36,1 58,3 48,3 48,5 50,9 65,2 • vir vergelykingsdoeleindes is die c-vsw-tellings omgeskakel na persentasie korrek en dit is gebaseer op die resultate van die kompeterende luistersituasies. • die hhie-prestasie is ook omgeskakel na die persentasie waarin geen probleme ondervind word nie en slegs die totale tellings word aangetoon. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 106 liza van wyk, isabel uys & maggi soer die linkerore en 48,5% vir die regterore. op die 5%-vlak van beduidenheid is groep 2 se prestasies vir beide ore beduidend swakker as die van groep 1 (linkerore: ρ = 0,0011; regterore: ρ = 0,0002). daar is egter nie 'n beduidende verskil tussen die prestasie van die linkeren regterore van elke groep afsonderlik nie (vir groep 1 is ρ = 0,1501; en vir groep 2 is ρ = 0,1378). groep 1 se waardes is hoer as 60% is en dui op geringe spraakdiskriminasieprobleme, terwyl groep 2 se resultate beduidend swakker as 60% is en dit dan op ernstige spraakdiskriminasieprobleme dui (p = 0,0353). 4. die bejaardes se subjektiewe evaluering van hul gehoorprobleme ten opsigte van die persentasie waarin die bejaardes geen probleme ondervind sonder en met gehoorapparate nie, behaal groep 1 onderskeidelik 56,3% en 76,8%. groep 2 behaal 50,9% sonder gehoorapparate en 65,2% met gehoorapparate. daar is dus 'n beduidende verskil tussen die resultate van elke groep, afsonderlik, sonder en met hul gehoorapparate. alle metings is uitgevoer op die 5%vlak van beduidenheid. ten opsigte van groep 1 se resultate is daar 'n beduidende verskil (p = 0,0015) op die totale prestasie met en sonder gehoorapparate! groep 1 se bejaardes is dus van mening dat die gebruik van gehoorapparate 'n beduidende bydrae lewer tot die kompensasie vir hul gehoorprobleme. ook ten opsigte van die emosionele(p = 0,0029) en die situasieveranderlikes (p = 0,0022) is daar 'n beduidende verskil sonder en met gehoorapparate. groep 2 se resultate dui ook daarop dat die bejaardes beduidend baat vind by die dra van gehoorapparate. net soos in die geval van groep 1 is daar 'n beduidende verskil tussen die prestasie sonder en met gehoorapparate vir die totale prestasie (p= 0,0010) en ook die metings van die emosionele(p = 0,0022) en situasieveranderlikes (p = 0,0010). op die 5%-vlak van beduidenheid is daar egter nie 'n beduidende verskil tussen die twee groepe se prestasie op die totale prestasie (p = 0,6331), die emosionele(p = 0,9834) en situasieveranderlikes (p = 0,2889) sonder gehoorapparate nie. die afleiding kan dus gemaak word dat beide groepe bejaardes se subjektiewe evaluasie van hul gehoorprobleem ooreenstem. ten opsigte van die evaluasie met hul gehoorapparate is daar nie 'n beduidende verskil op die totale prestasie (p = 0,1008) en die emosioneleveranderlikes (p = 0,3489) nie, maar daar is wel 'n beduidende verskil wat die situasieveranderlikes (p = 0,0201) aanbetref. groep 1 se resultate dui daarop dat hulle beduidend meer baat vind by die dra van gehoorapparate in verskillende luistersituasies as groep 2. hierdie resultate het belangrike implikasies vir die studie, aangesien dit die gebruik van objektiewe metings verder kan ondersteun. 5. die bejaardes se subjektiewe evalue* ring van hul gehoorprobleme teenoor die objektiewe meting van hul gehoorstatus en spraakdiskriminasievermoe resultate toon daar is 'n beduidende korrelasie tussen die ssi-toets en die aanbieding van die monosillabiese woorde en 'n ipsi-kompeterende boodskap (p = 0,000); tussen die vsw-toets en die aanbieding van die monosillabiese woorde en 'n ipsi-kompeterende boodskap (p = 0,031) en tussen die vswen ssi-toets (p = 0,000). die feit dat die toetsresultate korreleer, verhoog die toetsbetroubaarheid en -geldigheid van die studie. daar is verder 'n beduidende korrelasie tussen die bejaardes se subjektiewe evaluasie van hul gehoorprobleem en die objektiewe evaluasie van hul perifere gehoorstatus (p = 0,031). geen beduidende korrelasie bestaan egter tussen die bejaardes se evaluering van hul gehoorprobleme en hul spraakdiskriminasievermoe in verskillende luistersituasies nie. daar is geen beduidende korrelasie tussen die selfevaluasieskaal en die ssi-toets (p = 0,091); die vsw-toets (p = 0,934); en die aanbieding van die monosillabiese woorde met 'n ipsi-kompeterende boodskap (p = 0,2820) nie. geen beduidende korrelasie bestaan ook tussen die bejaardes se evaluering van die baat wat hulle vind by gehoorapparate en hul spraakdiskriminasievermoe in verskillende luistersituasies nie. daar is dus geen beduidende korrelasie tussen die prestasie met gehoorapparate en die ssi-toets (p = 0,839); die vsw-toets (p = 0,842) en die aanbieding van die monosillabiese woorde met 'n ipsi-kompeterende boodskap (p = 0,724) nie. bespreking bejaardes in groep 2 ervaar beduidend meer spraakdiskriminasieprobleme as die bejaardes in groep 1, ongeag die feit dat hulle suiwertoondrempels korreleer. hierdie resultate ondersteun die aanname dat spraakdiskriminasieprobleme 'n nie-perifere basis kan he en dus nie net verklaarbaar is op grond van suiwertoonresultate nie (marshall, 1981; rodriguez, et al., 1990). spesiale spraaktoetse is in hierdie studie gebruik om sentrale ouditiewe prosessering te ondersoek. die ssi-pi-funksie is swakker as die fg-pi-funksie (willeford, 1985). tydens die bepaling van die ssi-pifunksie het die "rollover-" fenomeen by sewe bejaardes van groep 2 voorgekom, terwyl hierdie fenomeen by slegs drie bejaardes voorgekom het tydens die bepaling van die fgpi-funksie. hierdie resultate kan aanduidend wees van aantasting van die ouditiewe senuwee of inperking ivan sentrale ouditiewe prosessering (kaplan, gladstorie & katz, 1984). j die tweede diagnostiese prosedure van die ssi-toets is om die ssi-ikben die ssi-kkb-resultate te vergelyk. met behulp van hierdie prosedure is dit moontlik om 'n sentrale letsel te identifiseer en is dit ook moontlik om te onderskei tussen breinstamletsels en temporale lobletsels (kaplan, et al., 1984). groep 2 het beduidend swakker gevaar as groep 1, maar albei groepe se resultate wyk ook af van die norme wat geld vir volwassenes (kaplan, et al., 1984). alhoewel geen bestaande norme beskikbaar is vir bejaardes nie, kan bestaande norme wel sinvol gebruik word om die bejaardes se resultate op 'n kwalitatiewe wyse te beskryf. afwykende resultate dui dus nie noodwendig op die spesifieke plek van letsel nie, maar kan wel aandui dat ouderdomsverwante veranderinge die funksie van die sentrale ouditiewe senuweesisteem negatief be'invloed (kaplan, et al,,1984; willeford, 1985). verder kom groter ikb-afwykings as kkb-afwykings by beide groepe voor (kaplan, et al., 1984). alhoewel hierdie resultate volgens die bestaande'norme op 'n moontlike breinstamletsel kan dui, kan hierdie afleiding nie noodwendig vir die bejaarde populasie gemaak word nie, the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) kdiskriminasie by bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers 107 v a n w e e die kompleksiteit van hul gehoorprobleme. afleidings kan wel gemaak word dat ouderdomsverwante v e r a n d e r i n g e wat voorkom in die sentrale ouditiewe s i s t e e m aanleiding kan gee tot die spesifieke uitvalle soos g e m e e t op hierdie toets by die bejaarde populasie (willott, 1991). die kwalitatiewe ontleding van die vsw-toetsresultate dui daarop dat bejaardes in beide groepe, meer probleme o n d e r v i n d in die kompeterende (veral links-kompeterende) luistersituasies (lukas & genchur-lukas, 1985;). hierdie resultate bevestig probleme in sentrale ouditiewe prosessering, wat onder andere manifesteer as geheue-, lees-, spelen spraakdiskriminasieprobleme. die resultate dui verder daarop dat bejaardes nie almal in dieselfde mate probleme met hierdie toetsprosedure ondervind nie. bejaardes wat dus swakker presteer in hierdie toets, se aantasting van die sentrale senuweesisteem blyk meer uitgesproke te wees as die bejaardes wat minder probleme ondervind. tydens die aanbieding van monosillabiese woorde en 'n ipsi-kompeterende boodskap behaal groep 2 swakker resultate as groep 1. hierdie resultate kan ook moontlik toegeskryf word aan die ouderdomsverwante veranderinge in die sentrale ouditiewe senuweesisteem, maar ook aan die degradering van die ouditiewe boodskap deur die gelyktydige aanbieding van 'n ipsi-kompeterende boodskap (marshall, 1981). wanneer die ouditiewe sein gekompliseer word en boodskapoortolligheid afneem, ondervind bejaardes probleme met die prosessering van 'n ouditiewe boodskap (marshall, 1981). die verskil in resultate van die twee groepe dui ook moontlik daarop dat die plek van aantasting in die ouditiewe sisteem verskil en bevestig ook die groter betrokkenheid van die sentrale ouditiewe senuweesisteem by groep 2 (penrod, 1994; stach, 1994). die meting van 'n bejaarde se gehoorverlies is slegs een komponent van die totale intervensieproses (ventry & weinstein, 1982; taylor, 1993). gehoortoetse kan wel 'n gehoorverlies kwantifiseer en spraakdiskriminasieprobleme identifiseer, maar dit bied nie voldoende inligting om die effek van die gehoorverlies op die bejaarde se alledaagse funksionering te jbeskryf nie. deur gebruik te maak van die bejaardes se subjektiewe evaluasie van hul gehoorprobleme kan waardevolle data ingewin word vir intervensie (cox, 1996; ventjry & weinstein, 1982). deur gebruik te maak van selfevaluasieskale kan die oudioloog inligting verkry oor die bejaarde se selfpersepsie van sy gehoorgestremdheid, wat deur bepaalde persoonlike, houdingsen situasiefaktore bei'nvloed word (taylor, 1993). op die langtermyn is dit tog die bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruiker self wat die sukses van die gehoorapparaatpassing beoordeel (cox, 1996). gevolgtrekkings en aanbevelings gesien in die lig van bogenoemde bespreking is dit duidelik dat die intervensie van bejaardes met gehoorprobleme veel meer behels as die evaluering van perifere gehoorstatus. hierdie populasie het unieke oudiologiese probleme en bepaalde behoeftes en in die lig hiervan kan die volgende gevolgtrekkings en aanbevelings geformuleer word: longitudinale navorsing kan gebruik word om die verloop van spraakdiskriminasieprobleme te monitor, en kan dus aanvullend tot die resultate van hierdie studie wees. alhoewel norms vir bejaardes nie beskikbaar is nie, beklemtoon hierdie navorsing die gebruik van spesiale spraaktoetse om spraakdiskriminasie ook in komplekse luistersituasie te evalueer. spesiale toetse moet dus gebruik word om die basiese toetsbattery aan te vul. die gebruik van selfevaluasieskale word sterk aanbeveel ten einde 'n holistiese beeld van bejaardes en bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers se ouditiewe funksionering te verkry. die resultate van die selfevaluasieskale is aanvullend tot die objektiewe toetse wat gedoen word. deur gebruik te maak van hierdie skale word die suksesvolle intervensie van bejaardes verder verseker. resente navorsing fokus op die sogenaamde "collaborative" (samewerkende) benadering (kricos, 1997). hierdie rehabilitasieprogramme is nie net gerig op die seleksie en passing van gehoorapparate nie, maar fokus ook op ander probleemoplossingstrategiee, soos die aanvaarding van en aanpassing by gehoorverliese, die gebruik van alternatiewe luistersisteme, spraakleesopleiding en die aanleer van ander kommunikasiestrategiee ten einde te verseker dat bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers effektief kan funksioneer in alledaagse luistersituasies (kricos, 1997). die resultate van hierdie studie kan ook aangewend word om die gehoorprobleme wat bejaardes ondervind, aan die familie van die bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruiker te verduidelik. familielede het dikwels beperkte insig in die hantering van en kommunikasie met bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers (kricos, 1997). deur ook opjeiding en inligting aan hierdie mense te verskaf kan die suksesvolle gebruik van gehoorapparate deur die bejaardes verder verhoog word (kricos, 1997). die insluiting van die meting van ouditief ontlokte potensiale kan ook in toekomstige navorsing gebruik word om die sentrale ouditiewe funksie van bejaardes te ondersoek (martin & cranford, 1989). resultate wat voorspruit uit sulke studies kan in verband gebring word met bestaande navor sings data en dus groter insig verskaf in die ouditiewe probleme van bejaardes. die gebruik van ouditief ontlokte potensiale, ten einde die sentrale ouditiewe funksie van bejaardes te evalueer, geniet tans beperkte aandag in suid-afrika en ook in die bestaande literatuur. bibliografie cohen, g. (1987). speech comprehension in the elderly: the effects of cognitive changes. british journal of audiology, 21, 221226. cox, r.m. (1996). the abbreviated profile of hearing aid benefit (aphab) administration and application. phonak fokus: news, ideas, high technology, acoustics, 21, 2-14. folstein, m.f., folstein, s.e. & mchugh, p.r. (1975). mini mental state examination: a practical method for grading the cognitive state of patients for the clinician. journal ofpsychian research, 12, 189-198. goodglass, h. & kaplan, e. (1983). the boston diagnostic aphasia examination. philadelphia: lea & febiger. hull, r.h. (1985). assisting the older client. in katz, j. (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. humes, l.e. & roberts, l. (1990). speech-recognition difficulties of the hearing-impaired elderly: the contributions of audibility. journal of speech and hearing research, 33, 726735. jacobs, c.d., haasbroek, j.b. & theron, s.w. (1992). effektiewe navorsing: navorsingshandleiding vir tersiere opleidingsdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 108 liza van wyk, isabel uys & maggi soer inrigtings. universiteit van pretoria: pretoria. jerger, j., jerger, s., oliver, t. & pirozzolo, f. (1989). speech understanding in the elderly. ear and hearing, 10(2), 78-89. kaplan, h., gladstone, v.s. & katz, j. (1984). site of lesion testing: audiometric interpretation, vol ii. baltimore: university park press. kricos, p.b. (1997). audiologic rehabilitation for the elderly: a collaborative approach. the hearing journal, 50(2), 10-19. laubscher, a.m.u. & tesner, h.e.c. (1966). enkellettergrepige woordelyste in afrikaans. ongepubliseed. universiteit van pretoria. lemme, m.l. & hedberg, n.l. (1988). auditory linguistic processing. in lass, n.j., mcreynolds, l.v., northern, j.l. & yoder, d.e. (eds.), handbook of speech-language pathology and audiology. toronto: b.c. decker, inc. lukas, r.a. & genchur-lukas, j. (1985). spondaic word tests. in katz, j. (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. margolis, r.h. & shanks, j.e. (1985). tympanometry. in katz, j.(ed.), handbook of clinical audiometry (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. marshall, l. (1981). auditory processing in aging listeners. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 46, 226-240. martin, d.r. & cranford, j.l. (1989). evoked potential evidence of reduced binaural processing in elderly persons. the hearing journal, 42(7), 18-23. muller, a.m.u. (1981). die afrikaanse sintetiese sinsidentifikasietoets. ongepubliseerd. oudiologie-afdeling: tygerberg hospitaal. penrod, j.p. (1994). speech threshold and recognition/ discrimination testing. in katz,j. (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (4th ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. rodriguez, g.p., disarno, n.j. & hardiman, c.j. (1990). central auditory processing in normal-hearing elderly adults. audiology, 29, 85-92. rosenberg, p.e. (1978). case history: the first test. in katz, j. (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (2nd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. stach, b.a. (1994). hearing aids and older people. the hearing journal, 47(3), 10-42. suid-afrikaanse buro vir standaarde. (1994). guide to national and international acoustics, electro-acoustics, vibration and ultrasonics publications. pretoria. taylor, k.s. (1993). self-perceived and audiometric evaluations of hearing aid benefit in the elderly. ear and hearing, 14(6) 390-394. theron, s.w. (1995). persoonlike gesprekke. departement menslike hulpbronbestuur. universiteit van pretoria. van wyk, m.e. (1997). spraakdiskriminasie by bejaarde gehoorapparaatgebruikers. ongepubliseerde m.kommunikasiepatologie verhandeling, universiteit van pretoria: pretoria. ventry, i.r. & weinstein, b.e. (1982). the hearing handicap inventory for the elderly: a new tool. ear and hearing, 3(3), 128-134. willeford, j.a. (1985). sentence test of central auditory dysfunction. in katz, j.(ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (3rd ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. willott, j. f. (1991). aging and the auditory system: anatomy, physiology and psychophysics. san diego: singular publishing group, inc. working group on speech understanding and aging. (1988). speech understanding and aging. journal of the acoustical society of america, 3, 859-895. yantis, p.a. (1994). puretone air-conduction threshold testing. in katz, j. (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (4th ed.). baltimore: williams & wilkins. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 44, 1997 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y o c t o b e r editonal 'j'he first summer school held by the south african logopedic society in september was a pronounced success. the course dealt with "dysaudia," and there is no doubt that the bringing together of audiologists, teachers of the deaf, audiometrists, ear, nose and throat specialists, speech therapists, and others bore fruitful results in terms of discussion and exchange of ideas. although we are unable, through lack of space, to print, the summer school papers in this journal, roneoed copies will be made available to those members who attended the course and to others on application. we are fortunate, however, in having an article in this issue by one of the speakers at the summer school, dr. haynes. it is rare that one is able to present an article by an ear, nose and throat specialist who has also made an intensive study of audiometries. the paper "the relation between speech therapy and psychotherapy" by joan philips is .likely to provoke comment. it certainly seems necessary to clarify the limitations of the speech therapist a s far a s psychological treatment is concerned. in "speech recording in stuttering therapy" by erica stern, a beginning is made of potentially important research. it is hoped that miss stern and others will continue to work in this field to our mutual benefit. finally, it is our real privilege to have amongst our contributors dr. m. k. wright. a little while' a g o dr. wright expressed the rather revolutionary opinion that speech therapists should be in a position to teach the neurologists how to classify aphasia. the present article is a culmination of an idea first thought of in 1949. it is a great stride towards a sounder and more useful analysis of l a n g u a g e disorders. peter rothenberg electrical and industrial instruments manufacturers of instruments under the registered trade mark "pret,j industrial and electro surgical instrument repairs 98c mooi street, johannesburg box 3904 phone 23-0730 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society june itoria during the past few years, members of the medical and dental professions, teachers, psychologists, etc., and also the public in general, have come to recognise and accept logopedics as an important and necessary element of the modern society in which we live. however, the scope of logopedics is still very much in the dark. the general impression seems to be that speech therapists deal only with speech defects, per se. λ pressing need exists to establish the fact that logopedicians, although generally known as "speech" therapists, are interested in and also in a position to treat patients with other problems. in this issue of the journal, articles indicative of the wide scope of logopedics, are presented, for example, reading difficulties, and also problems of language development in a brain-injured child, both of which, although not directly concerned with "speech" defects, nevertheless are within the speech therapist's realm, as they have as their basic essence, the factor of communication. finally, an article on recent research work done in the field of voice science is a further indication that the field of logopedics is wide and varied, and that the interest of speech therapists is not limited only to the more obvious speech defects. 54 wingate mansions cor smit & nugget streets, hospital hill, j o h a n n e s b u r g phone 44*0860 tpying and roneoing undertaken especially theses and students' notes. bessie dembo r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pg48-55.html communication after mild  traumatic brain injury – a spouse’s perspective samantha jayne crewe-brown alexandra maria stipinovich ursula zsilavecz department of communication pathology, university of pretoria correspondence to: a m stipinovich (alex_stipinovich@telkomsa.net) abstract individuals with mild traumatic brain injury (mtbi) often perform within normal limits on linguistic and cognitive assessments. however, they may present with debilitating communicative difficulties in daily life. a multifaceted approach to mtbi with a focus on everyday communication in natural settings is required. significant others who interact with the individual with mtbi in a variety of settings may be sensitive to communicative difficulties experienced by the individual with mtbi. this article examines communication after mtbi from the perspective of the spouse. a case study design was implemented. the spouses of two individuals with mtbi served as the participants for this study. semi-structured interviews were held, during which each participant was requested to describe the communication of their spouse with mtbi. the content obtained from the interviews was subjected to a discourse analysis. the results show that both participants perceived changes in the communication of their spouse following the mtbi. the results further show that mtbi affected communication of the two individuals in different ways. the value of a ‘significant other’ in providing information regarding communication in natural settings is highlighted. the implications of these findings for the assessment and management of the communication difficulties associated with mtbi are discussed. keywords: mild traumatic brain injury, spouse’s perceptions, communicative competence, discourse analysis, social interaction mild traumatic brain injury (mtbi) is said to account for the majority of patients admitted to hospital with brain injuries (cassidy et al., 2004), representing the greater population of all treated traumatic brain injuries worldwide (tay, ang, lau, meyyappan & collinson, 2010). to date, the majority of research in the area of traumatic brain injury (tbi) has focused on severe tbi (king, hough, walker, rastatter & holbert, 2006b). despite an increased awareness of mtbi, this therefore is the least understood form of brain injury (king et al., 2006b). the majority of behavioural research in the field of mtbi to date has been neuropsychologically based (duff, proctor & haley, 2002) with the impact of mtbi on communicative competence remaining largely unknown (whelan, murdoch & bellamy, 2007). mtbi is associated with a constellation of symptoms, including physical, cognitive, emotional and behavioural symptoms, that vary in terms of degree and rate of recovery after injury (tay et al., 2010). the cluster of symptoms following mtbi has been coined the post-concussive syndrome (pcs) (alexander, 1995) and has been reported in up to 50% of individuals who sustained an mtbi (satz et al., 1999). the majority of individuals who sustain an mtbi show spontaneous and complete post-injury recovery within a few weeks to a few months. however, some continue to present with symptoms after this time, with approximately 15% of these individuals complaining of disabling symptoms for as long as 1 year after sustaining their injury (alexander, 1995; duff et al., 2002). the most common symptoms encountered after mtbi may be grouped into three categories: (i) cognitive complaints (including difficulties with memory, attention and concentration); (ii) somatic complaints (including headache, fatigue and sensitivity to noise or light); and (iii) affective complaints (including depression, irritability and anxiety) (mcallister & arciniegas, 2002). as stated above, the majority of behavioural research in this field has been neuropsychologically based (duff et al., 2002). neuropsychological sequelae of mtbi have been found to include difficulties with reasoning, information processing, verbal memory and attention to detail, as well as slowed reaction time and reduced error recognition (kwok, lee, leung & poon, 2008; leininger, gramling, farrell, kreutzer & peck, 1990; voller et al., 1999). the frontal lobes of the brain are vulnerable to injury in tbi (mcdonald, flashman & saykin, 2002). given the role that these frontal regions play in the executive functions of regulating and organising behaviour, impulse control, self-monitoring, planning and reasoning skills, even individuals with mtbi demonstrate a strong tendency to exhibit executive dysfunction (mcdonald et al., 2002). impairment in executive abilities may have wide-ranging effects on an individual’s ability to function effectively in daily life and can impair job performance, activities of daily living and interpersonal relationships (mcdonald et al., 2002). furthermore, effective communication is reliant on cognitive skills, including attention, memory, word-retrieval ability, the formulation of thoughts, complex information processing and executive functioning (green, stevens & wolfe, 1997). even when the medical categorisation is ‘mild’, the effects of the brain injury may therefore have a severe effect on the person’s ability to communicate effectively (king et al., 2006b). barrow et al. (2003) warn that current methods of language testing might not provide sufficient cognitive load to expose the subtle difficulties that affect the functional performance abilities of individuals with mtbi. as a result, the identification of individuals with mtbi who might benefit from speech-language therapy remains tenuous. king et al. (2006b) agree, stating that testing procedures must incorporate tasks that are sensitive to the affected skills of individuals with mtbi. the administration of tasks of higher-order linguistic function demanding frontal lobe support has provided better insight into the language disorders associated with tbi (whelan et al., 2007). such tasks include reaction time measures examining speed and accuracy of naming, tasks requiring the organisation of substantial quantities of information, the processing of abstract language and the filtering out of environmental interference (barrow et al., 2003; king et al., 2006b; mathias, beall & bigler, 2004; whelan et al., 2007). recent research by whelan et al. (2007) aimed to profile the language abilities of an individual with mtbi using tasks hypothesised to demand frontal lobe support. difficulties were found in activities requiring complex lexical-semantic operations such as sentence construction, multiple definition formulation, absurdity detection or correction and passive/temporal structure completion (whelan et al., 2007). in addition, research by barrow et al. (2003), king, hough, vos, walker & givens (2006a) and king et al. (2006b) revealed compromised speed of word retrieval in individuals with mtbi during time-pressurised conditions. however, in contrast to the finding of word-retrieval deficits during confrontation naming, king et al. (2006b) found no significant deficits with regard to word retrieval in the discourse of participants with mtbi. they concluded that the increased cognitive load imposed by the speeded task of confrontation naming may have resulted in greater error occurrence than the discourse task did. daily work and home activities routinely involve time-pressured situations and multilevel processing (barrow et al., 2003). this may explain why individuals with mtbi who perform within normal limits on standard linguistic and cognitive assessments often present with debilitating difficulties in communicative and cognitive activities of daily life (kim et al., 2009; mcdonald et al., 2002). according to whelan et al. (2007), the real-life consequences of subtle cognitive-linguistic impairments after mtbi remain largely unexplored. the field of pragmatics is concerned with the communicative consequences of various cognitive and linguistic deficits on interaction (prutting & kirchner, 1987). pragmatic aspects of language are closely linked to judgements of a perceived level of social competence. social competence requires a complex repertoire of behaviours, including the integration of one’s knowledge of the world with cognitive, social, behavioural, psychological and linguistic processes (hartley, 1995). as communication involves the interaction of the individual with his/her environment, the environment in which communicative functions occur is considered a major determinant of communicative behaviour. for this reason, communication assessment and rehabilitation requires a multifaceted approach including a focus on everyday communication in natural settings (galski, tompkins & johnston, 1998; snow & ponsford, 1995). functional rating scales designed to determine the effect of deficits upon communicative activities and life participation are available. the pragmatic protocol (prutting & kirchner, 1987), for example, is a descriptive taxonomy designed to provide an overall communicative index for school-aged children, adolescents and adults. it consists of 30 pragmatic aspects of language and is completed by the clinician after observing 15 minutes of unstructured, spontaneous interaction between communication partners in a natural setting. according to lomas et al. (1989), the likelihood that clinician-assessors observe patients in true daily-living situations is slim. for this reason, rating scales that are reliant on reports made after direct observations by a significant other who spends substantial time with the client in a variety of settings are also available. the communicative effectiveness index (ceti) (lomas et al., 1989) is a functional communication measure for aphasia that gives the clinician first-hand evidence on the communicative performance of the individual with aphasia as observed by a significant other. this scale has also been found to provide a valuable measure of change in functional communication ability (lomas et al., 1989). a possible disadvantage to using a rating scale to examine communicative competence from the perspective of a family member, however, is that views are directly addressed as opposed to being carefully elicited. this may prevent unexpected and possibly valuable information from being disclosed. an alternative approach to gaining understanding of an individual within his/her environment is that of discourse analysis (da). da is a qualitative approach to the measurement of individuals’ perceptions (vyncke, 2000). in this approach, the content of the discourse obtained during a semi-structured interview is examined. as an individual’s choice of words to convey perceptions and experiences gives rise to individual versions of reality (willig, 1999), da provides valuable insight into an individual’s experience and interpretation of the topic discussed. in this study, da was used to examine the perceptions and experiences of spouses regarding the communication of two individuals with mtbi. in so doing, information regarding the impact of mtbi on communication in natural settings was obtained. method aim the aim of the study was to describe the communication of two individuals with mtbi from the perspective of the spouse. research design a case study design was selected within the framework of qualitative research. case studies aim to analyse a situation precisely and in detail in order to provide insight into the phenomenon being investigated (titscher, meyer, wodak & vetter, 2000). in this study, information was obtained from two participants through semi-structured interviews, the content of which was subjected to da. participant selection criteria the participants in this study were the spouses of two individuals with mtbi. trauma has been found to have different effects on individuals with differing kinship relationships (kreutzer, gervasio & camplair, 1994a; leach, frank, bouman & farmer, 1994). a common methodological limitation of previous research into family functioning after tbi is that data from relatives with different kinship relations are often combined (kreutzer, gervasio & camplair, 1994b). in this study, the perceptions regarding the communication of the two individuals with mtbi were therefore confined to the perceptions of the spouse. the participants were to be proficient in either english or afrikaans. they should have been living with their spouses for a minimum period of 1 year before the mtbi, ensuring familiarity with their spouse’s pre-morbid communication abilities. participants were to be residing with the individual with mtbi at the time of the interviews. a description of the participants (referred to as p1 and p2) is included in table i. selection and description of the individuals with mtbi two individuals with mtbi were selected according to the delineation of mtbi provided by the american congress of rehabilitation medicine (acrm) (1993). the individuals selected were therefore to have sustained traumatically induced physiological disruption of brain function, manifested by at least one of the following: loss of consciousness (loc) for up to 30 minutes; any loss of memory regarding events immediately before or after the accident; any alteration in mental state at the time of the accident; and focal neurological deficit(s) that may or may not have been transient. post-traumatic amnesia (pta) was not to have exceeded 24 hours after 30 minutes and the initial glasgow coma scale (gcs) score was required to have been 13 15, 30 minutes after the injury was sustained. the majority of individuals with mtbi show complete recovery of their symptoms within 1 3 months after injury (levin et al., 1987, in alexander, 1995). however, some exhibit persisting difficulties beyond 3 months after injury (e.g. alexander, 1995; kwok et al., 2008; leininger et al., 1990). a further selection criterion was therefore a post-injury interval of 3 months or longer to ensure that any change in communication associated with the injury would have stabilised and become part of the person’s communicative repertoire. as stated above, the individuals with mtbi were required to have been living with their spouses (the participants) for a year prior to the injury, and since the injury. the individuals with mtbi were selected by means of purposive sampling (strydom, 2005). patient records from a private hospital as well as patient records from the private practice of a neurologist were examined for individuals who met the selection criteria. information pertaining to duration of loc and duration of pta was not indicated in the hospital records. individuals were therefore initially selected based on the gcs score alone. information pertaining to loc and pta was obtained from the spouse. a description of the individuals with mtbi (referred to as mtbi1 and mtbi2) is provided in table ii. the spouse of mtbi1 is p1 and the spouse of mtbi2 is p2. as indicated in table ii, mtbi1 sustained a frontal lobe haemorrhage due to the injury. individuals with mtbi whose initial injuries include complications such as depressed skull fractures, contusions and subdural or epidural haematomas are more likely to have persistent cognitive deficits. however, the majority of such patients experience resolution of these symptoms, even if only after some delay (mcallister & arciniegas, 2002). mtbi1 was included in this study because his gcs score remained within the limits posited for the classification of mtbi, and he had sustained his injury 6 months prior to the interview. according to p1 and p2, loc was less than 30 minutes. according to mtbi1’s spouse (p1), mtbi1 presented with notable word-finding difficulties in the first week after the accident. however, she stated that he was orientated to place and person and that he was aware that he had been in an accident. research ethics the research was granted ethical clearance by the research proposal and ethics committee of the faculty of humanities, university of pretoria. the individuals with mtbi and their spouses were provided with a verbal and written explanation of the nature and purpose of the study, and gave written consent confirming their voluntary participation in the study. those who agreed to participate were assured of confidentiality at all times during the study. the participants were also free to withdraw from the study at any time. material and equipment for data collection a semi-structured interview was used to obtain the required data, which were then subjected to a da. da, as defined by willig (1999, p. 2) is concerned with, among other things, ‘the ways in which language constructs experiences …’. people use their discourse to construct versions of their social world (potter & wetherell, 1987). to adequately allow the participants to construct the individual versions of their social worlds, the interview attempted to create a ‘conversation encounter’, placing equal importance on the interviewee’s answers and the researcher’s questions (potter & wetherell, 1987, p.165). the researcher provided only two topic-introducing requests, attempting to guide the conversation rather than prescribe neutral and passive questions as in the case of a traditional interview (potter & wetherell, 1987). following the topic-introducing requests, the researcher proceeded with follow-up questions (e.g. ‘is that all?’), probing questions (e.g. ‘ok, tell me a little more about that’), specifying questions (e.g. ‘and how did you handle that?’), direct questions (e.g. ‘and for how long did that continue?’) and interpreting questions (e.g. ‘is that what you mean by …?’), so as to adequately understand the participants’ answers (kvale, 1996). the following two topic-introducing requests were presented within the interview: • request 1: ‘what do you think communication entails?’ communication is a multifaceted commodity encompassing non-verbal and verbal behaviour. individuals may differ in their judgements regarding appropriateness of social behaviour. cultural background, for example, shapes values, belief and stereotypes, and influences how individuals react with others (hartley, 1995). judgements made by the spouses regarding the communication of the individual with mtbi were likely to be made within the framework of their understanding of communication. by asking this question, attempts were made to obtain an idea of what communication meant for each participant and therefore what would be important for them with regard to their spouse’s communication. • request 2: ‘tell me about your spouse’s communication.’ in phrasing this request, no reference was made to communication difficulties. in this way, participants were free to comment on either positive or negative aspects of their spouse’s communication. no time frame in relation to the mtbi was specified in request 2. the omission of a time frame gave the participants the freedom to highlight or foreground any information pertaining to their spouse’s communication, within the context of their subjective understanding of communication that they felt was relevant at the time of the interview. as the participants were familiar with the individual with mtbi both before and after the accident, it was hoped that any changes that they may have noted in their spouse’s communication and which they ascribed to the mtbi would be spontaneously presented. where necessary, the follow-up, probing, specifying, direct or interpreting questions described above were asked in relation to the mtbi for the purpose of clarifying the participants’ responses. the conversations were recorded using an aiwa tp-510 cassette recorder and a hitachi vm e53e audiovisual cassette recorder for later analysis. procedure pilot study a pilot study was conducted to determine the clarity of the proposed topic-introducing requests, the adequacy of the recording equipment, and the time it would take to set up and conduct the interview. the selection criteria stipulated for the main study were used to select a single participant for the pilot study. the two requests posed were found to be understood by the participant. no changes to the questions or data collection and analysis procedures were therefore necessary. data collection procedure the interviews took place in the participant’s home or place of work. recording equipment was set up as unobtrusively as possible. the individual with mtbi was not present during the recording. the interviews ranged from 30 minutes to 1 hour in length. after completion of the interviews, the researcher analysed the discourse into themes according to the guidelines provided by potter and wetherell (1987) and wetherell, taylor and yates (2001). four weeks after the initial interviews, the participants were re-interviewed by the researcher to ensure that the data collected during the interviews had been interpreted correctly. the re-interview gave the participants opportunity to comment on the researcher’s interpretations (kvale, 1996). in this way, trustworthiness of the results was enhanced (de vos, 2002; lincoln & guba, 1985). data recording procedure the data (discourse) from the tape and audiovisual cassettes were transcribed in standard orthography in the relevant language so that the discourse could be easily analysed. data analysis procedure the content obtained during the interview was subjected to a da to obtain a qualitative description of the participant’s perceptions of the spouse with mtbi’s communication. da examines the content of the conversation, rather than aspects of structural organisation (jaworski & coupland, 1999). da therefore examines and interprets the meaning behind what is being said in the conversation. the transcription of the interview marks the start of the analysis process. the interviews were transcribed using standard english or afrikaans orthography. the discourse was read carefully by the researcher, as well as by a second professional with experience in da, thereby ensuring confirmability and trustworthiness (de vos, 2002; lincoln & guba, 1985). after reading the transcript repeatedly, the researcher recorded recurring images, words and issues next to the text in the first draft. the words and images used in the discourse were carefully studied and placed into categories based on similarities between them. themes were then identified within these categories according to the frequency with which they occurred, the information that followed regarding those themes, and the amount of discourse that was linked to each particular theme. patterns that occurred within themes were identified and viewed as sub-themes within the main theme. trustworthiness lincoln and guba (1985, p. 290) refer to the ‘truth value’ (or trustworthiness) of qualitative studies. measures implemented in this study to ensure trustworthiness included credibility, transferability, dependability and confirmability. credibility refers to the accuracy with which the participants are represented and described (lincoln & guba, 1985). to ensure credibility, the individuals with mtbi were selected according to the criteria provided by the acrm (1993). the individuals with mtbi, their spouses and the data collection and analysis procedures are described in detail. the semi-structured interviews were conducted in the participants’ first language to facilitate accurate expression and their responses recorded so that they could be transcribed word-for-word. transferability refers to the applicability of the findings to another context or group of people (lincoln & guba, 1985). the purpose of this study was not to generalise the findings to all individuals with mtbi. multiple variables influence the communication interaction of individuals, including those with mtbi. however, the analysis of communication following mtbi in natural settings is hoped to have relevance to other individuals with mtbi. dependability refers to whether or not the findings would be consistent if the enquiry were replicated (lincoln & guba, 1985). dependability was ensured by implementing a pilot study. furthermore, a follow-up interview was held with the participants to provide them with the opportunity to confirm the data and interpretations thereof. confirmability, also referred to as neutrality (de vos, 2002), emphasises the importance of the findings reflecting the questions posed by the study and the participants’ responses, rather than the researcher’s biases or prejudices. confirmability was facilitated by providing the opportunity for the participants to define communication. this definition, in turn, formed the context in which their description of the communication of the individuals with mtbi was interpreted. secondly, the omission of a time frame or reference to communication difficulties in the second topic-introducing question of the semi-structured interview ensured that their responses were in no way influenced by the researchers’ bias that mtbi may impact on communication. opportunity for the participants to confirm the data and the interpretations thereof was facilitated by the implementation of a second interview. a second observer assisted in the interpretation of the data, thereby strengthening confirmability. results and discussion participant 1 when asked for her view on what communication entails, p1 referred to communication as involving the verbal expression of one’s feelings: ‘... to express yourself ...’ ‘... to tell someone how you’re feeling ...’ p1 also recognised non-verbal components such as body language and facial expression: ‘... like they say “body language”...’ ‘... like how your body can talk and your facial expressions can also be part of communication ...’ p1 further emphasised communication for the purpose of interaction within the context of a relationship: ‘... you want to communicate with someone ...’ ‘... to tell someone how you’re feeling ...’ in summary, p1’s view of communication involved both verbal and non-verbal expression (including expression of emotions) within the context of a relationship. any changes in mtbi1’s communication that impacted on his verbal and non-verbal communication, or any changes in his communication that affected their relationship, were therefore likely to be noted by p1. when asked to describe mtbi1’s communication, p1 commenced by stating that he communicates well. she also mentioned that he is well liked and that he is an extrovert. however, throughout the interview process it became clear that she had noted numerous changes in his communication since the mtbi. three themes were identified in the da of p1’s description of mtbi1’s communication, namely loss of temper, word-retrieval difficulties and role change. loss of temper p1 stated that mtbi1 communicates well, but that he loses his temper more frequently since the accident: ‘no, he communicates well. it’s just that his temper ... he sometimes is a little short-tempered with the children ...’ ‘yes, he ... it’s [mtbi1’s temper] a lot shorter than it was before the accident.’ ‘... it’s just that he loses his temper extremely quickly ...’ the communicative consequence of feelings of anger or irritability may be loss of temper. loss of temper is therefore considered a form of communication. as p1 considered communication to entail the expression of emotions, any changes in mtbi1’s expression of emotions were likely to be noted by p1. according to p1, mtbi1’s loss of temper affected his ability to communicate and interact effectively with his family. his loss of temper appears to have affected p1’s relationship with him as well as his relationship with their children: ‘we fight a little more.’ ‘he sometimes becomes quite ugly with them.’ [the children] communication-related personality and psychosocial change has been found to profoundly influence an individual’s integration back into the family system (ylvisaker, szekeres & feeney, 2001). irritability and associated loss of temper is a common symptom in the first 3 months after mtbi, usually resolving thereafter (acrm, 1993). mtbi1’s loss of temper has persisted beyond 3 months. as stated, mtbi1 sustained a frontal haemorrhage. individuals with mtbi whose initial injuries include complications such as haemorrhage may be more likely to have persistent deficits (mcallister & arciniegas, 2002). emotional control requires a certain level of arousal. the reticular activating system (ras) influences the arousal level of the brain. the ras is sensitive to axonal damage owing to its multiple projections. decrements in arousal are therefore frequently associated with tbi, resulting in decreased cortical activation necessary for behavioural control. this, in turn, may result in irritability, poor frustration control and increased anger or rage (hartley, 1995). p1 mentioned factors that appear to trigger or contribute to mtbi1’s loss of temper. these include the use of alcohol, his children and his word-finding difficulties: ‘… if he has any alcohol in him then he gets angry, which wasn’t really the case before the accident.’ ‘if the children are here for just an hour or two he will “go off” at one of them …’ ‘… then he will give her a harder hiding than he ought to, or about something silly that doesn’t actually justify a hiding.’ ‘he basically gets angry if you don’t immediately know what he’s talking about.’ the ras, and therefore the brain’s arousal state, is influenced by stressors (including alcohol and anxiety). according to alexander (1995) individuals with mtbi have reported increased sensitivity to modest alcohol use. symptoms of pcs have been found to increase when individuals with mtbi are placed under stressful conditions (hanna-pladdy, berry, bennett, phillips & gouvier, 2001), resulting in inappropriate communication in stressful situations (ylvisaker et al., 2001). the environmental factors (children and alcohol) that are now considered by p1 to contribute to mtbi1’s loss of temper were present before his accident. it is possible that, since the mtbi, mtbi1’s cortical activation required for behavioural control in the presence of such stressors has been affected. mtbi1’s loss of temper may also be associated with executive dysfunction. the executive control centre is the point of integration of internal and external stimuli (hartley, 1995). executive functions within the control centre influence deliberate cognitive, social, academic, vocational and communicative behaviours (ylvisaker & feeney, 1998). executive dysfunction is a common consequence of mtbi owing to the high incidence of damage to the frontal lobes of the brain (kim et al. 2009). mtbi1 sustained a frontal haemorrhage. the possibility therefore exists that he sustained injury to his executive control centre, resulting in the excessive display of emotions, evident in his loss of temper. of significance in p1’s description of mtbi1’s loss of temper were her comments that he is remorseful once he has lost his temper with his children and that he does not exhibit this behaviour towards other people: ‘he says he often feels sorry right away … then he will say sorry, and then he feels very bad about it.’ ‘and he also won’t easily become aggressive with other people.’ mtbi1 therefore appears to possess some insight into his behaviour as well as the ability to perceive situations, to integrate these perceptions with stored knowledge, to determine a possible course of action and then to monitor his behavior in certain situations. these are all functions of the executive control centre (hartley, 1995). however, in the presence of stressors (such as alcohol or his children), he appears less able to exert control over his behaviour. mtbi1’s ability to exert control over or adapt his communication interaction to suit certain contexts and certain communication partners supports the notion that social competence cannot be interpreted unless communication and context are treated simultaneously (prutting, 1982). this, in turn has implications for the assessment of mtbi1’s communication and the identification of his loss of temper by a clinician. unless the clinician observes mtbi1 in the presence of the environmental stressors discussed above, the communicative consequence of irritability, manifested as loss of temper, may go undetected. the value of obtaining the subjective perceptions and reports of a significant other who spends time with the individual with mtbi in a variety of personally relevant settings is thus illustrated. word-retrieval difficulties in addition to mtbi1’s more frequent loss of temper, p1 reported that he experiences word-retrieval difficulties: ‘… and he sometimes forgets certain words, like when a person gets older… stupid little words, then he’ll, he won’t be able to get to the word. i’ve actually noticed that since the accident.’ ‘yes, it [word-retrieval difficulty] happens regularly. it actually happens a lot. it’s as if he can’t place the word, or remember the thing’s name.’ ‘... he searches for the word and then after a little while he’ll say, “man, there it is.” after two or three minutes the word will come.’ as p1 emphasised the importance of the verbal expression of how one feels in her definition of communication, it was likely that she would be sensitive to any change in mtbi1’s verbal communication. p1 further stated that the frustration experienced by mtbi1 when unable to express himself owing to these word-finding difficulties aggravated his loss of temper: ‘no, he becomes angry with himself. he can’t handle it [word-retrieval difficulty]. he doesn’t like it at all.’ ‘… he basically gets angry if you don’t immediately know what he is talking about.’ as word retrieval is a basic process in communication, a deficit in this area may significantly impact on an individual’s overall communicative ability (king et al., 2006a). word-retrieval difficulties typically affect communication by slowing interaction and by increasing hesitations and pauses in discourse (hartley, 1995; ylvisaker et al., 2001). a number of studies have assessed word retrieval following mtbi (e.g. barrow et al., 2003; king et al., 2006a; king et al., 2006b). deficits in word retrieval were found to be associated primarily with increased time taken to retrieve words. these deficits were ascribed to centralised cognitive slowing and reduction in the supervisory function governing control, memory and initiation processes (barrow et al., 2003; king et al., 2006a; king et al., 2006b). as indicated by the quote above, p1 also indicated that with time mtbi1 is able to access the target word. king et al. (2006b) found that participants with mtbi exhibited no significant difference when compared with non-injured control participants regarding word retrieval during discourse tasks. a formal tool of word finding in discourse was used in the study by king et al. (2006b). it was argued that the cognitive load imposed by the discourse task was not sufficiently high to detect the subtle word-retrieval difficulties exhibited by the same participants during the confrontation naming task (king et al., 2006b). in contrast to these findings, word-retrieval difficulties during conversation were reported by p1. discourse that takes place during stressful work or home activities involving multilevel processing may constitute sufficient cognitive load to elicit word-retrieval difficulties. this, again, has implications for clinician-based assessments and highlights the value of obtaining reported observations of a significant other who interacts with the client in a variety of settings. role change in her definition of communication, p1 highlighted the importance of communication within a relationship. in her description of mtbi1’s communication, she indicated a change in their relationship since the accident, with her having to assume a parent role: ‘… after the accident i took the role of being a parent. like he was almost like one of the children.’ ‘so basically he’s now almost lost his role.’ communication is ongoing and cyclical. communication interactions or relationships evolve over time as the communicative behaviour of one individual interfaces with that of another within an ongoing situation (hartley, 1995). the range of physical, cognitive and behavioural difficulties exhibited by the injured individual may result in a loss of peer-based and reciprocal relationships, with the spouse often being forced to take on a parental role (kreutzer et al., 1994a). role change experienced by spouses of individuals who have sustained a tbi has been reported in the literature and is considered to be a reason for the increased stress, depression and anxiety experienced by the spouse (e.g. kreutzer et al., 1994a; leathem, heath & woolley, 1996). in summary, p1 reported changes in mtbi1’s communication since his accident. these changes included more frequent loss of temper, particularly in the presence of environmental factors including children and the use of alcohol. she also reported word-retrieval difficulties, as well as a change in roles with her having to assume the role of parent. valuable information regarding mtbi1’s communication was provided by p1 which might not have been obtained through the administration of formal tools in unnatural settings, or even through clinician-based observation. participant 2 when asked to describe her view of what communication entails, p2 placed emphasis on verbal expression: ‘for me talking stands above everything.’ p2 also made reference to communicative behaviour as reflecting one’s attitude: ‘... your attitude of how you behave and what you radiate as a person.’ like p1, p2 referred to communication within the context of relationships: ‘you must have good communication to build up a good relationship ...’ given p2’s views on communication, she was likely to be sensitive to any changes in mtbi2’s verbal expression, attitude and behaviour or the influence of these changes on his relationships. when asked to describe mtbi2’s communication, p2 stated that her husband likes to speak and that he likes to express his emotions: ‘he likes to talk. he likes to express his emotions, by talking and also by showing how he feels. he’s not someone who likes to keep things to himself.’ throughout the interview process, p2 made reference to numerous changes in mtbi2’s communication interaction since the accident. three themes were identified in the da of p2’s description of mtbi2’s communication, namely adynamia, memory loss and social withdrawal. adynamia according to p2, there was a change in mtbi2’s drive and motivation since the accident. she described this change in the following way: ‘he is still a perfectionist, but he’ll sometimes still say, “ag no, i don’t feel like doing that now.” that little spark that should be there is no longer there.’ ‘he doesn’t have that motivation.’ ‘the driving power is gone.’ ‘because he’d go and sit and sit still for hours and do nothing, but it didn’t bother him …’ ‘yes, and that’s not how he was. he was always busy, always kept busy.’ a lack of drive or motivation may not be considered by everyone to constitute a communication difficulty. however, in her description of what communication entails, p2 stated that communication represents one’s attitude and behaviour. therefore, any changes in mtbi2’s behaviour or attitude were likely to have been noticed by p2 and reported by her in her description of his communication. lack of motivation, as described by the term adynamia , often occurs in individuals who have been affected by tbi (hartley, 1995). the basal ganglia and their connections to the limbic system are regions in the brain that are involved in motivation. because of the likelihood of damage to the anterior and mesial temporal lobe (part of the limbic system) and to the basal ganglia, changes in motivation and emotional responses within subcortical and limbic input are common after tbi (hartley, 1995; ylvisaker et al., 2001). despite having sustained a mild brain injury, mtbi2 reportedly also exhibited changes in motivation. individuals with executive dysfunction may also exhibit adynamia (hartley, 1995). the frontal lobes modulate and regulate the expression of internal drives and affective states. depending on the nature of their injury, individuals with executive dysfunction may be unable to formulate and initiate goal-directed behaviour, to the point where expression of emotion or desire is lacking (hartley, 1995). as stated, executive dysfunction is a common consequence of mtbi and can have far-reaching effects on ability to function in daily life, on job performance, and on interpersonal relationships (kim et al., 2009; mcdonald, et al., 2002). in the case of p2, mtbi2’s loss of drive and motivation affected their relationship. she emphasised that mtbi2 has made progress with regard to his ability to start something and finish it. however, this change in her husband remained an adjustment for her: ‘ag, yes, it’s going a lot, he’s probably actually quite fine now to me, it was more the first year and a half was a bit, you could notice it [adynamia] easily … it was sometimes very frustrating.’ ‘but it’s sometimes just an adjustment for a person from how he was to what he was then.’ [after the accident] memory loss p2 reported a change in mtbi2’s memory since the mtbi: ‘yes, you know, with regard to memory, this is actually a thing he ... he genuinely always had a memory like an elephant, but i don’t know, these days you can tell him something and he will swear high and low that you didn’t tell him.’ ‘he still has blanks and at times there are things he cannot really remember.’ ‘yes, ag, things like places we’ve been to ... yes, small silly things that he generally would have remembered.’ residual memory problems in mtbi typically resolve after 3 months. however, impairment in retention can persist (levin, 1989). the effect of memory loss on communication includes slowed interaction, repetition in conversations and social breakdown (ylvisaker et al., 2001). although individuals with mtbi often score within normal limits on standard memory tests, close relatives frequently report considerable everyday memory problems (kim et al., 2009). this again supports the notion that information regarding the injured individual’s functioning in daily, natural settings is essential in the identification of difficulties that may go undetected on formal testing. memory difficulties in individuals with mtbi may be associated with medial temporal or diencephalic pathology (kim et al., 2009). however, individuals with injury to the frontal lobes may also exhibit disruptions in the memory process and of the functions that facilitate memory. this is because the executive control centre controls memory processes by generating strategies to enhance storage and retrieval of information (hartley, 1995). social withdrawal p2 described mtbi2 as having become withdrawn in the first 18 months following the mtbi, participating less in social interactions: ‘... with regard to his communication, he was withdrawn directly after the accident. he was quieter ...’ ‘yes, so he became a little more of an introvert ... and he didn’t speak much at that stage ...’ ‘it was frustrating at times because he had always spoken a lot and always said how he felt and what he was thinking and explained his reasoning ...’ the reasons for mtbi2’s reduced social interaction after his accident are unclear. this social withdrawal may be a consequence of his adynamia, or lack of motivation. motivation influences an individual’s attentional processes and thereby affects social interaction by either dampening or stimulating efforts at processing appropriate responses (prigatano, 1987). parker (1996) also reported that reduced motivation after tbi can impair efforts at social interest within individuals. in her definition of communication, p2 emphasised that in addition to verbal expression, behaviour also contributes to an individual’s communication. the possibility exists that mtbi2’s adynamia affected his behaviour, resulting in social withdrawal. in summary, when viewed from the perspective of p2, mtbi2’s communication interaction appears to have been affected by adynamia, memory loss and social withdrawal. p2 stated that there have been improvements with regard to mtbi2’s drive and motivation as well as his social interaction, and that these two aspects of his communication were most problematic in the months following the accident. his memory difficulties, however, appear to have persisted over the 3 years following his accident. table iii summarises the themes identified from p1 and p2’s perceptions of their spouses’ communication after mtbi. conclusion the results of this study show that the brain injury was perceived by both participants to have affected communication interaction of the individuals with mtbi. the results further show that the perceived effect of the injury on communication was not the same for each individual with mtbi. in certain cases, the communicative consequences of the mtbi (for example, the frequent loss of temper reported by p1) appeared to be the product of the interaction between the organic pathology and environmental factors. these findings are seen to have numerous implications for the field of speech-language pathology. firstly, the range of communicative difficulties reported by the participants in this study necessitates the involvement of the speech-language therapist (slt) in the assessment and management of communication of individuals with mtbi. individuals with mtbi usually return to work and are expected to perform at the same cognitive-communicative level as they did before their injury. the communicative demands placed on them are often higher than those placed on individuals with moderate or severe brain injuries. the slt has a role to play in educating hospital staff as well as family members, colleagues and employers of individuals with mtbi regarding possible consequences of mtbi that may result in communication problems. a second implication of this study pertains to the identification by the slt of communication difficulties in individuals with mtbi. the word-finding difficulties described by p1 and the memory difficulties reported by p2 might have been identified during the administration of standardised cognitive-linguistic assessment tools, provided that the cognitive load of the assessment tasks was high enough to expose subtle difficulties in these areas. however, the more frequent loss of temper reported by the one participant, for example, is considered a product of the interaction between the (mild) organic pathology and environmental factors. the need for contextually relevant assessment procedures that focus on communication in natural and personally relevant settings is highlighted. a third implication pertains to the management of communication difficulties associated with mtbi. in this study, information was obtained not only regarding communication of the individuals with mtbi, but also on the effect of these difficulties on interpersonal relationships and functioning within natural settings. this, in turn, has implications for the compilation of personally and contextually relevant management programmes by slts. furthermore, the complex nature of the difficulties reported necessitates the involvement of a team in the management of individuals with mtbi. the use of formal assessment procedures as well as functional rating scales is lacking in this study. inclusion of such procedures would have facilitated triangulation of the data. it is recommended that future research into the communication of individuals with mtbi in natural settings should include a greater number of participants as well as a broader range of assessment approaches. furthermore, investigation into the perceptions of the individual with mtbi him/herself regarding the impact of the injury on communication would provide additional valuable information. in conclusion, the results of this study suggest that individuals with mtbi may present with communication difficulties that are evident in their natural environments. these findings support the notion that communicative competence in a range of personally relevant settings needs to be considered in the assessment and management of the communication difficulties associated with mtbi. references alexander, m.p. (1995). mild traumatic brain injury: pathophysiology, natural history and clinical management. neurology, 45, 1253-1260. american congress of rehabilitation medicine [acrm] (1993). definition of mild traumatic brain injury. journal of head trauma rehabilitation, 8, 86-87. barrow, i.m., hough, m., rastatter, m.p., walker, m., holbert, d., & rotondo, m.f. (2003). can within-category naming identify subtle cognitive deficits in the mild traumatic brain-injured patient? journal of trauma injury, infection and critical care, 54(5), 888-897. cassidy, j.d., carroll, l.j., peloso, p.m., borg, j., von holst, h., holm, l., et al., (2004). incidence, risk factors and prevention of mtbi: results of the who collaborating centre task force on mtbi. journal of rehabilitation medicine, 43 (supplement), 28-60. de vos, a.s. (ed.) (2002). research at grass roots (2nd ed.). pretoria: van schaik publishers. duff, m.c., proctor, a., & haley, k. (2002). mild traumatic brain injury (mtbi): assessment and treatment procedures used by speech-language pathologists (slps). brain injury, 16(9), 773-787. galski, t., tompkins, c., & johnston, m.v. (1998). competence in discourse as a measure of social integration and quality of life in persons with traumatic brain injury. brain injury, 12, 769-782. green, b.s., stevens, k.m. & wolfe, t.d.w. (1997). mild traumatic brain injury: a therapy and resource manual. san diego: singular publishing group, inc. hanna-pladdy, b., berry, z.m., bennett, t., phillips, h.l., & gouvier, w.d. (2001). stress as a diagnostic challenge for postconcussive symptoms: sequelae of mild traumatic brain injury or physiological stress response. the clinical neuropsychologist, 15(3), 289-304. hartley, l.l. (1995). cognitive communicative abilities following brain injury: a functional approach. california: singular publishing group, inc. jaworski, a., & coupland, n. (1999). introduction: perspectives on discourse analysis. in a. jaworski & n. coupland (eds.). the discourse reader. london & new york: routledge. kim, s.-j., kim, o.-l., seo, w.-s., koo, b.-h., joo, y., & bai, d.-s. (2009). memory dysfunction after mild and moderate traumatic brain injury: comparison between patients with and without frontal lobe injury. journal of the korean neurosurgical society, 46, 459-467. king, k.a., hough, m.s., vos, p., walker, m.m., & givens, g. (2006a). word retrieval following mild tbi: implications for categorical deficits. aphasiology, 20(2/3/4), 233-245. king, k.a., hough, m.s., walker, m.m., rustatter, m., & holbert, d. (2006b). mild traumatic brain injury: effects on naming in word retrieval and discourse. brain injury, 7(20), 725-732. kreutzer, j.s., gervasio, a.h., & camplair, p.s. (1994a). primary caregivers’ psychological status and family functioning after traumatic brain injury. brain injury, 8(3), 197-210. kreutzer, j.s., gervasio, a.h., & camplair, p.s. (1994b). patient correlates of caregivers’ distress and family functioning after traumatic brain injury. brain injury, 8(3), 197-210. kvale, s. (1996). interviews: an introduction to qualitative research. california: sage publishers. kwok, f.y., lee, t.m.c., leung, c.h.s., & poon, w.s. (2008). changes of cognitive functioning following mild traumatic brain injury over a 3-month period. brain injury, 22(10), 740-751. leach, l.r., frank, d.e., bouman, d.e., & farmer, j. (1994). family functioning, social support and depression after traumatic brain injury. brain injury, 8(7), 599-606. leathem, j., heath, e., & woolley, c. (1996). relatives’ perceptions of role change, social support and stress after traumatic brain injury. brain injury, 10(1), 27-38. leininger, b.e., gramling, s.e., farrell, a.d., kreutzer, j.s., & peck, e.a. (1990). neuropsychological deficits in symptomatic minor head injury patients after concussion and mild concussion. journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, 53, 293-296. levin, h.s. (1989). memory deficit after closed-head injury. journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 12(1), 129-153. lincoln, y.s., & guba, e.g. (1985). naturalistic inquiry. california: sage publications. lomas, j., pickard, l., bester, s., elbard, h., finlayson, a., & zoghaib, c. (1989). the communicative effectiveness index: development and psychometric evaluation of a functional communication measure for adult aphasia. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 54, 113-124. mathias, j.l., beall, j.a., & bigler, e.d. (2004). neuropsychological and information processing deficits following mild traumatic brain injury. journal of the international neuropsychological society, 10, 286-297. mcallister, t.w., & arciniegas, d. (2002). evaluation and treatment of postconcussive symptoms. neurorehabilitation, 17, 265-283. mcdonald, b.c., flashman, l.a., & saykin, a.j. (2002). executive dysfunction following traumatic brain injury: neural substrates and treatment strategies. neurorehabilitation, 17, 333-344. parker, r.s. (1996). the spectrum of emotional distress and personality changes after minor head injury incurred in a motor vehicle accident. brain injury, 10(4), 298-302. potter, j., & wetherell, m. (1987). discourse and social psychology: beyond attitudes and behaviours. london: sage publications. prigatano, g.p. (1987). personality and psychosocial consequences after brain injury. in m. meier, a. benton & l. diller (eds.). neuropsychological rehabilitation. new york: guilford press. prutting, c.a. (1982). pragmatics as social competence. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 47(2), 123-134. prutting, c.a., & kirchner, d.m. (1987). a clinical appraisal of the pragmatic aspects of language. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 52(2), 105-119. satz, p., alfano, m.s., light, r., morgenstern, h., zaucha, k., asarnow, r.f., & newton, s. (1999). persistent post concussive syndrome: a proposed methodology and literature review to determine the effects, if any, of mild head and other bodily injury. journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 5(21), 620-628. snow, p., & ponsford, j. (1995). assessing and managing changes in communication and interpersonal skills following tbi. in j. ponsford (ed.). traumatic brain injury rehabilitation for everyday adaptive living. east essex: lawrence erlbaum associates. strydom, h. (2005). sampling and sampling methods. in a. s. de vos, h. strydom, c. b. fouché & c. s. l. delport (eds.). research at grass roots: for the social sciences and human service professions (3rd ed.). pretoria: van schaik publishers. tay, s.y., ang, b.t., lau, x.y., meyyappan, a., & collinson, s.l. (2010). chronic impairment of prospective memory after mild traumatic brain injury. journal of neurotrauma, 27, 77-83. titscher, s., meyer, m., wodak, r., & vetter, e. (2000). methods of text and discourse analysis. london: sage publications. voller, b., benke, t., benedetto, k., schnider, p., auff, e., & aichner, f. (1999). neuropsychological, mri and eeg findings after very mild traumatic brain injury. brain injury, 13(10), 821-827. vyncke, i.o. (2000). discourse of subjects with early onset and late onset parkinson’s disease: a linguistic and motor speech profile. unpublished d.phil. dissertation. johannesburg: university of the witwatersrand. wetherell, m., taylor, s., & yates, s.j. (2001). discourse as data: a guide for analysis. bath: bath press. whelan, b., murdoch, b.e., & bellamy, n. (2007). delineating communication impairments associated with mild traumatic brain injury: a case report. journal of head trauma rehabilitation, 22(3), 192-197. willig, c. (1999). introduction: making a difference. in c. willig (ed.). applied discourse analysis: social and psychological interventions. buckingham: open university press. ylvisaker, m., & feeney, t. (1998). collaborative brain injury intervention: positive everyday routines. california: singular publishing group. ylvisaker, m., szekeres, s., & feeney, t. (2001). communication disorders associated with traumatic brain injury. in r. chapey (ed.). language intervention strategies in aphasia and related neurogenic communication disorders (4th ed.). philadelphia: lippincott williams & wilkins. table i. description of participants p1 p2 gender female female primary language afrikaans afrikaans number of years of education 14 14 current occupation technologist landscaper occupation at time of spouse’s mtbi technologist landscaper number of children three none home environment house in residential area house in residential area table ii. description of individuals with mtbi mtbi1 (p1’s spouse) mtbi2 (p2’s spouse) date of injury 27 september 2003 24 february 2001 time since injury at time of first interview 6 months 3 years glasgow coma scale (gcs) score, according to hospital records 14/15 13/15 duration of loss of consciousness, according to spouse <30 minutes <30 minutes duration of post-traumatic amnesia, according to spouse orientated to place and person within 24 hours, but presented with notable word-finding difficulties for 4 5 days <24 hours number of years married to/living together with spouse 5.5 years 4 years primary language afrikaans afrikaans number of years of education 12 12 occupation prior to mtbi unemployed (previously a supervisor in a retail business) landscaper employed at the time of interview? no yes table iii. summary of the themes identified from the participants’ perspectives of communication after mtbi p1 p2 loss of temper adynamia word retrieval difficulties memory loss role change social withdrawal the syndrome of early infantile autism g . j. newstadt, m . b . , b . c h . (rand), d . p . m . (rand).* department of psychiatry & mental hygiene, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg and department of psychiatry, johannesburg hospital . . . but it has been delayed till i am indifferent, and cannot enjoy it; till i am solitary and cannot impart it . . . dr. samuel johnson. psychosis in children is not common. although it had been recognized since the early 20th century, it was not until 1943 that kanner first described the condition known as early infantile autism. the reasons for this delayed recognition are implicit in the manifold changes which occur normally in the young child. young children may show wide variations in mobility, speech, mood and learning ability, and it is precisely these fluctuations which render differentiation from normality difficult. psychosis is a conflict between the ego, or self, and reality. it follows therefore that the psychotic child shows some aberration or withdrawal from reality. in his original paper, kanner felt that the condition was a specific form of childhood psychosis characterized by profound withdrawal and lack of contact from the very first years of life, an obsessional demand for sameness in the environment, a lack of communication in the use of language, and a preference for relationships with inanimate objects. today autism is regarded as a syndrome occurring in early childhood, usually before the second year of life, and is characterized by an apparent difficulty in family social relationships. it is possible to delineate a pure or nuclear form in which no evidence of brain damage is demonstrable, and a mixed form in which elements of other diagnosable conditions are present. a third or borderline group exists which contains children who show features of the autistic syndrome, without gross social withdrawals i.e. some dysphasias, agnosias and apraxias. prevalence. although no adequate epidemiological studies have yet been published, small surveys in the united kingdom have suggested that one or two children out of every three thousand births are affected. the incidence is 3 to 4 times higher in boys than in girls. it is rare, though possible, to find more than one autistic child in a family. clinical features according to mildred creak, the following nine points describe items of behaviour commonly seen in children showing autistic behaviour. * present address: department of psychiatry, albert einstein college of medicine, yeshiva university, bronx, new york, u.s.a.' journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 12, no. 1 : september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e syndrome of early infantile autism 7 normal children can show any of the behaviour mentioned at some time in their lives. autistic children, however, show these patterns for years on end and to the exclusion of any other. the oddness of the autistic child is discernible all day and every day. 1. gross and sustained impairment of emotional relationships with people. this shows itself in: (a) the child's aloof and distant manner. the child behaves as though its fellow human beings did not exist, unless he is approached by people he likes. (b) persistent tendency to turn away from people or look past them when spoken to. (c) autistic children are never cuddly and never respond to being picked up. although parents may claim that they can "get through" to their autistic child, the children always lack outward signs of warmth towards people who do not know them. 2. self-examination. preoccupation with parts of his body (i.e. hands and feet) long after the baby stage, with a tendency to examine these objects as though they had appeared spontaneously, is commonly seen. 3. preoccupation with objects, or certain characteristics of them, without regard to their accepted function persisting long after the baby stage. this may be shown in one or more of the following ways: (a) collecting objects of all kinds to carry around and showing great anger if any one is lost. (b) great attachment to one special object such as a box, a piece of cloth etc., with distress if such an object is lost. (c) making lines and patterns with objects regardless of their real use. (d) tendency to examine objects in peculiar ways e.g. listening to, biting on, and scrutinizing from peculiar angles. (e) odd play with objects e.g. spinning them, flicking bits of string. 4. sustained resistance to change in the environment and a striving to maintain order or sameness. t h i s may show itself in one or more of the following: (a) great difficulty in changing routines and severe reactions to even minute changes. (b) resistance to learning new things. (c) great distress if familiar objects such as furniture are changed. 5. behaviour leading to suspicion of abnormalities of the special senses in the absence of any obvious physical cause. this may show itself in one or more of the following ways: (a) speech: i. no reaction to speech or voice. ii. positive attempts to get away from some noises which occasion distress. iii. apparent deafness. (b) vision: i. no reaction to things seen. ii. some interest in moving objects but little interest in stationary objects. t y d s k r i f v a n die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 12, nr. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 g. j. newstadt ' iii. positive attempts to get away from some objects seen, iv. apparent blindness or short-sightedness. (c) apparent indifference to pain or thermal changes. (d) willingness to taste unusual objects with or without food faddiness. 6. abnormalities of mood. these may show in one or more of the following ways: (a) outbursts of violent and prolonged rage and distress with screaming, tears, stamping, kicking, biting, etc., brought about by change of routine, a special fear, interference by others, or for no discernible reason at all. during these outbursts the child cannot be comforted even by someone he knows and loves. (b) periods of laughing and giggling, for which the reasons may be obscure. (c) lack of fear of real dangers. 7. speech disturbances. these may show in one or more of the following ways: (a) no speech at all, either from birth or it may have begun and been lost. (b) fragments of speech and contractions of words. (c) persistent simple speech as for a two year old. (d) reversal of pronouns e.g. "me" for "you", "he" instead of " i " . (e) parrot-like repetitions of words, phrases, sentences or even long poems and songs without regard to meaning. (f) frequent use of a special voice different from that of the normal one, sometimes with special peculiarities of pronunciation. (g) strange pedantic type of speech. in general the child has difficulty in communicating all but the simplest of his needs by means of speech. he may prefer to use gestures in order to show what he wants instead of asking. if these measures fail to achieve his goal, he may take people by the hand and lead them to the desired object.. 8. disturbances of movement and general activity. these may show in one or more of the following ways: (a) great overactivity, with or without sleep disturbances. (b) immobility. (c) special movements which may include rocking, head banging, jumping, twisting, flapping, writhing, spinning, facial grimacing, odd ways of walking, unusual movements of hands, repetition of the same movements, and extreme pleasure in bodily movement such as swinging, rocking, riding in cars, etc. 9. a background of serious retardation in which islets of normal, near normal or exceptional intellectual function may appear. this means that, on the whole, the child is well behind his age group in performance. unlike his normal counterpart of the same age, the autistic child requires his mother's supervision all or most of the time. in contrast to this extreme inadequacy he shows himself to be more than usually capable of performing certain tasks such as calculations, puzzles, singing and remembering music, reading and writing, memorizing long lists of dates and names, poems, odd facts, etc., even if oblivious of the meaning. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 12, no. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e syndrome of early infantile autism 9 aetiology there is no known illness or injury which automatically produces this behaviour and no one knows why various conditions should be accompanied by autism in some children and not in others. schools of thought in this state of uncertainty many theories tend to flourish, but two schools of thought may be delineated. firstly, there are those who believe the cause to be sociological, and secondly, there are those who believe that the primary cause is organic. it seems to the author that the two theories are not mutually exclusive, and it is probable that both views are correct; the organic or sociological factors expressing themselves to different degrees in individual cases. 1. psychodynamic and sociological. from the sociological viewpoint as typified by kanner and rank, autism is the mother's and/or parents' inability to create a warm emotional climate—the frigidaire atmosphere— which prevents the child's ego from developing its capacities for the externalization and taming of the drives. the result is, in rank's words, 'a fragmented ego'. the personality structure of these children represents fragments of various stages of development showing high achievement of some of the executive functions of the ego, while other manifestations of the ego or instinctual drives remain crippled or on a much lower level. other workers believe that early infantile autism represents a fixation at, or a regression to, the most primitive phase of extrauterine life, and the most conspicuous symptom is that the mother, as representative of the outside world, seems not to be perceived by the child. fundamental to the condition is a primary inability to distinguish between lifeless and living objects, as well as an inability to-distinguish the self from inanimate objects in the environment. parental attitudes are thought to be of some importance in the genesis of the condition. fathers of autistic children are described as often being highly intelligent with academic careers, but cold, detached, obsessional, and unable to form warm relationships. some workers have found that almost all such parents are grossly disturbed, inconsistent, hypochondriachal, pseudodelinquent, or even psychotic. in this regard goldfarb believes that the speech disturbance seen in these children, is largely determined by abnormal speech models seen in the parents. 2. genetic and constitutional factors. there is no factual evidence available to support the thesis that autism is an inherited condition, although the frequent occurrence of similar traits in the progenitors is suggestive of a hitherto unexplained hereditary element. this contrasts with adult schizophrenia in which simple genetic mechanisms are demonstrable. autism probably bears little relation to schizophrenia as seen in the adult. 3. physical causes. autism may be associated with definite organic diseases, but aside from these, current investigations suggest that hormonal, t y d s k r i f v a n die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 12, nr. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 g. j. newstadt biochemical, neurological and perceptual abnormalities may be of primary significance, and this field is presently being explored. the foregoing aetiological factors are not entirely convincing, but it must be borne in mind that methodological problems are aggrevated by the relative rarity of the syndrome, by varying therapies employed, and by adequate evaluation of the outcome, which is rendered difficult by the long period (years) required for follow up. differential diagnosis the diagnosis of autism often requires the expert services of a child psychiairist but florid cases are difficult to miss. however, the following conditions must be distinguished from autism. 1. mental subnormality. here there is usually uniform retardation in verbal and performance ability but some cases may show features of autism. unevenness of mental development so common in autistic children is rarely seen in defective children. 2. obsessional neurosis. in older children, severe obsessional neurosis or the rare condition of schizophrenia of the adult type may stimulate autism. 3. problems of deafness or aphasia. these can often prove difficult to exclude since autistic children rarely co-operate with objective testing procedures. careful observation by the parents can help to establish the diagnosis. deaf children are usually normal in other respects, although they may also show autistic behaviour, in which case there is little purpose in differentiating them from 'nuclear' autistic children. 4. conditions causing brain damage. brain damage, regardless of aetiology, may be associated with autistic behaviour, but signs of organicity such as spasticity, or epilepsy are usually associated. treatment at present, there is no treatment which can cure a child who shows autistic behaviour. it is therefore necessary to have realistic goals in therapy, without developing therapeutic nihilism. much can be done to help these children develop their potentialities, and compensate for their disabilities. the main aims in therapy are: (a) to modify general behaviour until the child is socially acceptable even if a little odd by ordinary standards. (b) to extend the range of motor and verbal abilities and to increase understanding of everyday life, so that the child learns to care/for himself. (c) to teach the child skills which will enable him to gain employment and earn a living in open, or, if necessary, sheltered employment. (d) to develop any special talents that the child may have, and to widen his knowledge, so that he can find interest in life. (e) to exploit his potential so as to make him an integral part of the community. medical advice should be sought as soon as possible after the diagnosis is suspected. therapy should naturally be directed towards the entire family. it journal of the south african logopedic society, vol 12, no. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e syndrome of early infantile autism 11 is important, however, that mothers should have some time to themselves, in view of the great burden placed upon them by such children. in the realm of speech therapy much can be done to correct and improve the child's powers of communication. vocalization should be encouraged and the therapist must be careful to use speech suitable for the child's level of understanding, so as not to discourage him. at the same time his understanding and vocabulary may be increased by the gradual introduction of new words and phrases whenever he shows some spark of interest. it may fall within the speech therapist's realm to help correct the distorted body image, commonly accompanying autism, and the familiarizing of the child with parts of its body may be essential. the child should, wherever possible, be maintained in its own hoirie, and institutionalization should be avoided. s u m m a r y the theoretical orientation, symptomatology, aetiology, and management of early infantile autism has been reviewed. the important role of psychiatrist, speech therapist and parents, in the therapy of this condition is stressed. it would seem that although the cure of autism can rarely be accomplished, early diagnosis and adequate therapy can produce improvement in many cases, and above all, prevent aggravation of the condition by a failure to understand it. opsomming die teoretiese orientasie, simptomatologie, etiologie en hantering van vroee, infantiele outisme word bespreek. die belangrike rol van die psigiater, spraakterapeut en ouers in die terapie by so 'n toestand word beklemtoon. alhoewel outisme selde genees kan word, kan vroegtydige diagnose en toereikende terapie tog verbetering in baie gevalle bewerkstellig en belangriker nog, verswakking van die toestand deur 'n gebrek aan begrip, uitskakel. references i. bender, l . (1947): childhood schizophrenia amer. j. orthopsychiat., 27, 68. 2 browne, i. (1965): problems of infantile autism in biochemical approaches to ' mental handicap in children london: ε & s livingstone & co. ltd. 3. creak, m . (1961): preliminary report of workshop on autistic children british medical journal, i i , 899. 4. creak, m. (1963): autistic children british journal of psychiatry, 109, 84. 5. creak, m. (1960-1961): report to the society for autistic children. 6. esman, a. h. (i960): childhood psychosis and childhood schizophrenia amer. j. orthopsychiat., 30, 391. 7 goldfarb, w., braunstein, d „ lorge, i. (1956): a study of speech patterns in a group of schizophrenic children amer. j. orthopsychiat, 30, 391. 8. goldfarb, w., braunstein, d., scholl, m. (1959): the speech of schizophrenic children amer. j. orthopsychiat., 29, 481. 9 mahler, m. s „ furer, m., settlage, c. f. severe emotional disturbances in childhood in american handbook of psychiatry new york: basic books. t y d s k r i f v a n die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 12, nr. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 g . j. newstadt 10. meyers, d., goldfarb, w. (1962): psychiatric appraisals of parents and siblings of schizophrenic children amer. j. psychiat., 118. 902. 11. rayner, e. w. (1963): childhood schizophrenia leach, 33, 130. 12. schulman, j. l . (1963): management of the child with early infantile autism amer. j. psychiat., 120, 250. 13. singer, μ . t . , wynne, l . c. (1963): differentiating characteristics of the parents of childhood schizophrenics, childhood neurotics and young adult schizophrenics amer. j. psychiat., 120, 234. 14. wing, l . autistic children london: national association for mental health. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 12, no. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society relationships between stammering and aphasia by marion fleming, f.c.s.t. although i have been a practising speech therapist for over 30 years, have had a wide variety of experience in treating stammerers of many ages and types, have read and studied as widely as possible in a busy professional life, i have never yet heard or read an explanation of the aetiology of stammering which is even remotely satisfying. it cannot be denied that no method of treatment has yet been found which is successful in every case, or even in more than a comparatively small percentage of cases. nevertheless, new theories are constantly put forward and each advocate claims to have discovered the treatment par excellence for this strange malady which has been known to exist for many centuries. the cause has been attributed to faulty breathing — the "cure," therefore, being breathing exercises, etc. the cause has been attributed to weakness of the tongue — the cure being intensive exercises or even, in the middle ages, surgical operation or the use of some mechanical device for supporting the tongue during speech. there have been psychological explanations by the dozen — emotional problems of the individual being blamed for the "conflict between the desire to speak and the desire to remain silent" (macdonald ladell) ( 1 ) . the speech difficulty has been attributed to a discrepancy between the rate of thought and the rate of utterance(2); to the lack of "complete maturation for highly-corticalized, one sided gradient for smooth verbal expression"*3*; to a difference in the speed of impulses sent out from the right and left hemisphere; and (taking a further step towards the neurological rather than the psychological explanations which have held the field for so long) to some slight undetected lesion in the central nervous system, probably in the region of the corpus striatum. cluttering has been defined as "a speech defect wherein excessive rapidity, slips of the tongue, iterations and indistinctness predominate" (a. moolenaar-bijl)(4). the confusion and the distinction between clutteriug and true stammering still, however, remains somewhat vague — leopold stein in treating of cluttering writes as follows: "the over-rapid speech and re-iteration bring to mind disturbances of speech caused by diseases of the mid-brain." he compares it to the type of speech which is "found in patients suffering from organic diseases of the striopallidar system (e.g. encephalitis lethargica, pseudobulbar palsy). occasional fits of weeping and outbursts of temper seem to support this assumption. the impairment of the co-ordination between mentation and verbalisation is the distinctive feature of some types of aphasia."(5) in another chapter he makes the following statement: "numerous investigations of the stammerer's constitution which are, however, not yet sufficiently complete, emphasize the striking frequency of signs relating to an inferiority of the lower brain centres (strio-pallidum, thalamus, hypothalamus) and the vegetative nervous system."(8) these opinions intensify rather than reduce, the confusion which prevents a clear-cut division between cluttering and stammering. the distinctions made by freund and quoted by stein in his book are as in figure i. in the opinion of the writer, many of these statements are only valid in a limited number of cases and cannot be taken as a general guide. what does emerge is that whatever the underlying organic condition may be, the resultant disorder is intellectual in nature. it is in the formulation of thought that the fundamental imperfection is found. consequently the most satisfactory explanation of stammering would seem to be that which was propounded by c. s. bluemel, namely that hesitation in speech is due to a corresponding hesitation in thought. his particular method of treatment is not one which would appeal to every speech therapist nor be likely to succeed with every stammerer — there are too many other complications to be considered in relation to stammering. nevertheless, as an explanation of the fundamental "predisposition" which has been postulated in vague terms for many years, it would seem to be more satisfactory and is certainly more definite than most of the explanations which have been put forward up to date. the psychologists have held the field for many years with their assumptions that stammering as a symptom can be explained in terms of emotional trauma of every variety — ranging from interference with the patient's native handedness (causing resentment), jealousy, neglect, over-protection, etc. to marital disharmony in the adult. the "precipitating" factor is considered to range from severe illness, frights and shocks of various kinds, to such emotional trauma as are listed above. speech being an expression of thought and emotion, why should such experiences seize particularly upon speech unless there is a fundamental weakness in the ability to formulate thought. if one wishes (unconsciously) to cease to hold communication with one's r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society november fellow-men, why not a hysterical aphonia rather than stammering? if one merely wishes (unconsciously) to escape the hurly-burly of life or a transitory unpleasant experience, why not "nervous dyspepsia" or a hysterical paralysis? if one wishes (unconsciously) to attract attention and prominence, there are other ways of doing so than by stammering. it seems reasonable to assume that the underlying cause upon which the neurosis is able to seize, is an inherent weakness in verbal imagery. this leads us to a consideration of "specific abilities." again there does not appear to be any satisfactory explanation of the vast differences which are found in any group of society. in regard to intelligence woodworth says the following:— "differences in intelligence are shown to depend in some slight degree, at least, on the stimulation received from the home and school environment, but most of the variation of intelligence in the population remains unaccounted for unless it be by heredity. intellectual sex differences are slight and are largely dependent on social custom, but the most striking, like the girl's superiority in language, and the boy's in mechanical interest, may depend on heredity."(8) vant to our subject if the above quotation read: "siblings of identical heredity will select different environments," etc. in considering the stammerer one is considering in many cases the "odd man out" — the person who hesitates and stumbles in a family of fluent speakers or maybe the one who has simply gone one step further in a rather inarticulate family. he may be a stammerer in a family of stammerers. the "specific ability" in relation to speech, may be undeveloped just as in many people artistic ability is undeveloped for reasons which probably cannot be explained any more than the manifestation of genius or special talent can be explained. it does not seem possible to account neatly and definitely for individual variations in specific ability — "ability resides in organic structure, but as we cannot practically observe a living man's brain structure our evidence of his abilities always comes down to a matter of observed or probable performance."(10) miss kingdon-ward in her book on stammering (1941) makes the statement that "any thorough study of stammering must include some study of aphasia." let us then examine the usual conceptions of aphasia and try to discover any connection between these and the preceding remarks on awareness of the disorder concentration on the disorder speech in the presence of strangers speech when at ease brief definite answers repetition of at first faulty sentences essay writing, spelling stammering yes yes worse better difficult often worse normal figure i. cluttering no no better worse easy better mostly equally faulty (7) we start here from the premise that girls possess greater language ability than boys, and it is wellknown that the incidence of stammering is far higher in boys and men than in girls arid women. but while heredity and environment are considered to play equally important parts in the development of specific abilities, there is again no clear-cut explanation to account for individual differences in children who are subject to the same hereditary and environmental influences. woodworth gives the following, which goes part of the way only:— . . . "each individual selects his own environment in large measure and siblings of different heredity will select different environments. they prefer different toys, companions, radio programmes and reading matter, and so expose themselves to different influences. they are sure to be treated differently for it is practically impossible to treat two persons alike if they differ in intelligence or personality."(9) it would be more relestammering. head defined aphasia as "a disorder of symbolic formulation and expression" the symbols in this case being words — words heard, words read, words uttered, words written, words used as tools in the formulation and expression of the person's thought. there have been many classifications of the different forms of aphasia — the basic one being, of course, the division into sensory and motor, receptive and expressive (or executive). head's further subdivision, although it is now considered too limiting and not truly descriptive of the many manifestations found in aphasic patients, still remains for the writer the most helpful as a general guide both to diagnosis of individual symptoms and to treatment. his four categories are as follows:— verbal aphasia — "defective power of forming words, whether for external or internal use." nominal aphasia — "a disturbance in the use of words as names and a difficulty in appreciating r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society the nominal significance of words" syntactical aphasia — "lack of that perfect balance and rhythm necessary to make the sounds uttered by the speaker easily comprehensible to his auditor." semantic aphasia — "characterised by want of recognition of the ultimate significance and intention of words and phrases apart from their direct meaning." (the writer has frequently found that patients who have made considerable recovery from one of the first three, are left with the inability to grasp the full significance of a passage — i.e. the semantic form). in the aphasic all the manifestations are caused by damage to the brain, to the "organic structure" mentioned above, either to the cortex itself or to associative fibres linking one area of the cerebrum with another. according to the beliefs of some neurologists, knowledge of the site of the lesion will indicate the type of language impairment which may be expected. how then, does all this link up with our hypothetical view of the basis of stammering? is it not permissable to assume that in the person who stammers certain cells have failed to develop to the normal extent, there is incomplete maturation of the central nervous system — a weakness which deviates so slightly from the normal that it may not even be revealed until the person is subjected to some particular stress or strain or great fatigue. supposing the child who stumbles and stammers in his eagerness to tell his mother: "i saw a b-b-bunny rabbit" has really some weakness in verbal imagery which makes him akin to the aphasic suffering from an inability to remember names — nominal aphasia. the usual explanations of stammering, of becoming conscious of the sound " b " , speech-conscious, worried about speech, etc., would still hold good, but what of the hundreds of children who are subjected to similar influences and "wrong handling" by parents and still come through the experience unscathed. one might, in the same way, compare the aphasic suffering from syntactical aphasia to the stammerer who (quite apart from the actual stammer) has great difficulty in expressing himself in words. the stammerer who is incapable of clear thinking, of giving definitions, who is amnesic and loses the thread of his discourse — has a kinship with the semantic aphasic. finally the extra-ordinary contortions of the speech organs performed by many stammerers in their attempts to utter are akin to the apraxia of the mouth which is seen in so many patients suffering from "verbal" aphasia. has apraxia been clearly explained except in terms of the outward form? one can find other correspondences — for example, there is the stammerer who expresses himself fluently in writing where the act of uttering is difficult; there is the aphasic in whom the power to paper paper paper paper paper paper paper paper paper paper paper paper is the third most important commodity in the world! transvaal paper & manufacturing co. ( p t y . ) ltd. i specialists in 1 s t a t i o n e r y scholastics, transparent tapes, pads and envelopes, printings, i . j i n i i v m l n i . typing papers, pen-carbon books, accounting books, carbons. 9 w r a p p i n g krafts, tissues, greaseproof, twines, tapes, bags all shapes and • sizes "for butcher, baker and candle-stick maker." i ο n o m f s t i r serviettes, d'oyleys, plates, toilet tissues, drinking cups, o . w n i i v > ι iv-. shelving papers, etc. 53 simmonds street phone 33-7723 j o h a n n e s b u r g . r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society november write is relatively unimpaired where the power to utter is grossly impaired; there is the stammerer whose speech is more fluent in reading aloud than in conversation; there is the aphasic to whom the printed word conveys considerably more meaning than the spoken word. finally let us consider the type of stammer which is often manifested in aphasics who have made a considerable degree of recovery. this is generally labelled a pseudo-stammer and attributed to a lack of co-ordination between brain and muscle. it may be likened to the attempts of an unskilled carpenter to hit a very small nail on the head with a very small hammer — he is unlikely to aim perfectly the first time he tries. the same applies to any skill — to the golfer or tennis player learning to hit the ball, to the sculptor chiselling a block of stone. woodworth draws a distinction between a man's ability and his capacity. may not the skill i.e. the "capacity," which is destroyed in the aphasic, be innately lacking in the stammerer, just as the ability of a henry cotton or a "little mo" or an epstein to develop a special skill is innately lacking in me. in conclusion i should like to describe one patient whose symptoms stimulated me to think on these particular lines. she is a woman aged 48, happily married with three healthy children. the stammer is reputed' to date from the age of 3, but became noticeably worse about the age of nine when it was attributed by a psychiatrist to unconscious jealousy of a more brilliant and attractive sister. when she was referred to me for treatment, the stammer was of the clono-tonic variety. it does not affect dny particular consonants and is non-variable. she is an artist by profession, in a very unusual individual medium, and her visual sense is highly developed. she is also intensely musical. her auditory sense is, therefore, developed in one direction but upon investigation it was found thai her sense of words is exceptionally poor. she has no real interest in them acoustically as musical sounds, and her vocabulary is poor in relation to her cultural background. her memory span for words is abnormally short. it was found that she had great difficulty in remembering more than three nonsense syllables spoken in sequence. we gradually trained her to repeat five with effort. she also complained that she could not remember telephone numbers for even a few seconds. we trained her first of all to repeat telephone numbers, then car index numbers. after that we proceeded to poetry, first of all selecting poems with very short lines. she had difficulty in repeating even one short line, but after some weeks of practice became able to repeat a verse of four lines. gradually, we introduced poems with longer lines. the next step was to read short stories asking her to re-tell them in her own words. she had great difficulty in doing this, but improved with practice. we also set her to define various objects or words and to paraphrase proverbs and the like, but she invariably described such activities as "torture." during this time we were constantly building up her confidence and helping her with the emotional difficulties which had grown up around her speech. she reported that when she was nervous on social occasions she "reversed her sentences." she is undoubtedly one of the relatively few patients i have known whose speech is infinitely better when she is paying particular attention to it. these are random thoughts upon a controversial subject and it may be that my conclusions are nonsensical. on the other hand it may be that they will stimulate others to further investigation on these lines or provide a missing link in investigations which have already been begun. it is certainly upon these lines that the writer intends to carry out research. if these conclusions are true, the treatment of stammering will be to some extent revolutionised and undoubtedly simplified. the psychiatrist, the neurologist and the speech therapist would work in .close co-operation, each making a contribution, for, whatever may be the ultimate origin of stammering, the influence of emotional trauma will always play an important part. references: (1) t h e stammerer unmasked by r. macdonald ladell — 1940. (2) speech and voice by leopold stein — 1942. (3) speech pathology by lee e d w a r d travis — 1931. (4) article on cluttering in " t w e n t i e t h century speech and voice correction" edited b y emil froeschels — 1948. (5) speech and voice b y leopold stein — 1942. (6) ibid. (8) p s y c h o l o g y : a study of mental life b y r. s. w o o d worth (18th edition) — 1946. (9) ibid. (10) ibid. macmillan publishers st. martins st., london, w.c.2 • "trippingly on the tongue" by mona swann. 1955. lod. • "forward to drama"— book i by g. h. holroyd. 1956. 6s. 6d. • "spoken english" by a. g. mitchell. 1957. 7s. 6d. • "an approach to choral speech by mona swann. 1956. 4s. 6d. • "english speech rhythm in theory and practice" by b. lumsden milne. 1957. 4s. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some reflections on the genetics of mental disorder by ingram f. anderson. m.b.b.ch.(rancl) department of medicine and clinical genetics unit, johannesburg general hospital. "heredity, the only one of the gods whose real name we know . . . brings gifts of strange temperaments . . and impossible desires." — oscar wilde. a study of aberrant mental mechanisms involves consideration of the interaction of the soma, psyche and multiform environment. such a vast field cannot be surveyed critically or even adequately in a dissertation of this nature. the title of the paper is thus employed advisedly: "some" because the scope is necessarily limited, "reflections" because here are mirrored my thoughts and those of others which may not be true images, "genetics" indicates etiologic restriction and "mental disorder" is used in the broad sense. the average medical-man, like the man in the street, has very little occasion to come up against cases of mental abnormality. the latter are locked away early — out of harm's way, out of society's way, out of medicine's way . . . however most of the major advances in genetics and especially in cytogenetics in the last few years have evolved against this very background and notably within its parameters, as exemplified in the case of mongolian idiocy. the recent splurge of spiders across the erstwhile neatly-lined pages of the medical journals has seemed to many to represent the soap-bubble interest of a narrow speciality. on the contrary, the implications have been widespread and diverse, and have impinged upon the whole broad structure of medical-biology. it is noteworthy that the layman has always been aware of an hereditary component in mental illness, as evidenced by everyday statements such as, "there's madness in x's family. comes through the father's side it does!" the medical world has been tardy in accepting such a situation and only in the last few decades has it given concrete formulation to the concept. the current explosion of activity and interest in human genetics has again focussed attention on the domain of the psychiatrist. whereas psychiatric genetics previously appeared to be an empiric exercise and its applications in mental disease were thought to denote a state of irreversibility, it now provides a point of vantage for research and therapeutic application. thus a genetically determined biochemical disturbance may not only point the way to new means of diagnosis but offers possibilities of correction at the molecular level. with these introductory remarks it will be propitious to pass on to a consideration of (a) mental deficiency and (b) the psychoses. (a) mental deficiency. about one out of every thousand whites in this country is in a mental asylum because of mental deficiency. the problem comprises elements of considerable philosophic and social importance; it provokes the question of why people are different and what determines their individuality; it limelights the paradox of human rights and raises the polemic aspects of eugenics as these reflect in the mirror of human ecology. almost half of the institutionalised cases are simple oligophrenics. so-called simple mental deficiency involves the genetics of intelligence. the latter is a tenuous entity, dependent on multifactorial inheritance and it is safe to say that its precise nature remains a matter of controversy. one reads in the book of job: "but where shall wisdom be found? and where is the place of understanding? man knoweth not the price thereof; neither is it found in the land of the living. the deep sayeth it is not in me and the sea sayeth it is not with me. it cannot be gotten for gold, neither shall silver be weighed for the price thereof. whence then cometh wisdom? and where is the place of 'understanding? . . . " for our purposes, we note that the simple moron has no distinctive characters and statisically lies at the negative end of the journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gaussian distribution of intelligence. in contrast to this somewhat ubiquitous entity, great strides have been made in our understanding of two definitive types of mental defect, namely, mongolism and phenylketonuric oligophrenia. (1) mongolism. it is convenient to discuss this in terms of a collocation of the three time factors in the biologic picture. on the largest scale is evolution, with man the result of a long line of ancestors and himself the potential ancestor of a long line of descendants. the interest here is historic. when langdon-down published his classic paper on "an ethnic classification of idiots", in 1866, he described mongolism for the first time, characterising the mongol as, " . . . a representative of the great mongolian race". crookshank in 1931 added the hypothesis that these cases were derived from mongolian ancestors and further that they represented an atavistic return towards the orang-utang. there is no scientific validity for this idea and the anomaly can be readily recognised in the mongol race itself. because of this confused inference attendant on the name, the designation langdon-down syndrome has been preferred by many. on the intermediate time-scale we consider the individual as such — his life history. mongolism exceeds all other morbid conditions of severe mental retardation in number, constituting some 5 to 10% of asylum inmates. there are approximately 125 mongols at the institution at witrand. the incidence in the population is nearly one in every 600 births. as a comparative measure it is salutory to note that the incidence of hypertensive disease is 5%, of all forms of congenital heart disease about 1%, while myelomatosis accounts for 3 per 100,000 of the population. ι the child with mongolism is usually born into a normal family with parents and siblings ofjten above average intelligence. an enormous j psycho-social problem is at once generated \ and cannot be elaborated upon here. apart from the mental subnormality, mongols jexhibit extensive physical abnormality,, proneness to infection being quite a feature. the advent of the antibiotic era has seen a rise in the mean survival age of the mongol. on the smallest time scale are events at the dynamic intracellular level. interest here centres on the chromosome constitution. in man each cell contains 46 chromosomes: 22 pairs of autosomes and a pair of sex-chromosomes. in 1959 lejeune and his co-workers discovered that the mongol possesses an extra autosome — a state of aneuploidy (i.e. an abnormal number of chromosomes). basically two mechanisms underlie the chromosomal aberrations in mongolism: non-disjunction and translocation. non-disjunction is the common type. during meiosis there is failure of separation of two homologous chromosomes. thus one daughter cell will come to contain both components of an autosome pair and the other cell neither. the former, after fertilisation will be trisomic (i.e. it will have three instead of two of a certain chromosome, in this case number 21 on the denver classification), and will have a diploid number of 47. this situation is seen typically with older mothers and may be related to mechanical factors operating in an old ovary with "old" ova. less commonly translocation occurs. this involves the breakage of two non-homologous chromosomes and an exchange of fragments between them. the greater part of chromosome 21 becomes attached to one or other fragment leading to a normal diploid number of 46, but in fact there exists a virtual trisomy 21. in addition translocation allows of the formation of a carrier state and the defect may therefore be perpetuated through several generations. in essence then, whatever the mechanism, an additional chromosome is overtly or occultly present. a great many genes — each controlling specific metabolic processes — lie a'on^ its length. however, the relationship of the cytology to either the biochemistry or to the pathology remains obscure. it is important to realise that the mongol is abnormal from the moment of conception. a degree of developmental irreversibility is attendant at birth and therapy must therefore be more or less ineffective, t h e r e is no rationale for the use of thyroid extract, alpha glutavite, siccacell therapy (injection of dried foetal brain cells) or large doses of vitamin e. (2) phenylketonuria. a somewhat brighter vista is revealed when we look to the inborn error of metabolism, phenylketonuria. this is the paradigm of a genetically determined endecember, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) zyme-block. it is transmitted as a single autosomal recessive, the homozygous state being necessary for phenotypic expression. the enzyme, phenylalanine hydroxylase, is at fault and there follows failure of oxidation of the essential amino-acid, phenylalanine, to tyrosine. various intermediate products accumulate proximal to the block and act deleteriously on the nervous system causing profound mental subnormality. treatment takes the form of a phenylalanine-low diet, and if commenced immediately after birth, offers a reasonable chance of preventing the mental disturbance. although the heterozygotes are mentally and physically quite normal, they can be detected by means of blood tests (the phenylalanine-load test) and this fact can be utilised in eugenic counseling. finally, very simple tests are available for diagnosis and have been employed routinely in many maternity centres overseas. this logical application in preventive medicine could be profitably employed in this country. (b) the psychoses. whereas the mental defective lies outside our conceptional world and symbolises all the horror of mute animalism in man, the psychotic may be regarded as a rather interesting eccentric with whom one can still maintain a somewhat narrow contact and communication. professor l. a. hurst stands uniquely and eloquently in relation to psychiatry in south africa as the protagonist of the hereditary discipline in this field. his researches, disciples and numerous publications on the subject testify cogently to his dynamic influence in a school formerly dominated solely by the environmentalists. he has moreover succeeded in impregnating a matrix of genetic precepts not only into the crude clay of his own speciality, but into the ground-substance of related medical divisions. the endogenous psychoses have been shown by the work of f. j. kallmann in america and eliot slater in britain, to be determined on an autosomal hereditary basis. the genetic mechanism is single recessive in schizophrenia and irregular dominant in manic depressive psychosis. one has illustrated in the case of phenylketonuria how a mutant gene, via the enzyme-block hypothesis, creates a disturbance of cellular chemistry ultimately manifesting in grave mental disintegration. the same basic process is believed to underlie the endogenous psychoses, but it is far more subtle, as yet elusive and cannot be so elegantly demonstrated. it is central to the understanding of the etiogenesis of these psychoses to appreciate the remarkable shift in emphasis implicit in the foregoing statements. for, whereas such mental diseases have been attributed to traumatic psychological experiences, which cause a disturbed and maladjusted psyche, it is now postulated that there is a primary genetic error underlying and indeed creating a disruption at the intracellular level: an imbalance in internal neuronal metabolic milieu. without elaborating on the detailed biochemistry and in broad terms, mental mechanisms depend upon the interaction of four (or more) endogenous neurohormones, adrenalin, nor-adrenalin, acetylcholine and serotonin. a delicate state of homeostasis between these substances is a pre-requisite for mental normality — imbalance is believed to underlie the genesis of the psychoses considered here. interestingly enough, many of the drugs used as therapeutic agents have been found to act upon and interfere with the biochemical patterns of the aforementioned neurohormones and have thus furnished further evidence for the indictment of these latter substances in the etiology of the psychoses. professor hurst is a strong advocate for the employment of such therapeutic agents as the monoamine oxidase inhibitors (e.g. iproniazid) and the chemically unrelated substance, imipramine in the pin-pointing of the peccant chemical sequences. while much remains to be propounded and still more remains to be clarified, it is apparent that the kaleidoscopic conundrum of the endogenous psychoses has assumed a new colour and format in terms of basic genetic concepts and the elaboration of the latter in the direction of enzyme chemistry, neurochemistry and neuropsychopharmacology. while these "reflections" have been rather terse and incomplete, they do highlight the emergent fact of an operative genetic component in many forms of mental disorder. armed with this evidence our knowledge can be translated into practical application in the form of eugenic counseling. a clinic for the provision of this service to the community has been inaugurated at the johannesburg general hospital under the aegis of 2 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) professor hurst. genetic advice is furnished in a wide variety of circumstances, not least of all in cases of mental disorder. present policy is in line with similar clinics in europe and america: the odds are stated, the genetics of a particular disease is explained and no attempt is made to influence parents as to whether or not they should have children. however, the role of the counselor is not entirely passive as one emphasised at the recent pretoria congress on genetics: "with regard to the counselor, these very developments in cytologic and biochemical genetics have altered the milieu in which he acts, in as much as his prognostications change in emphasis from passive empiricism to definitive understanding in terms of enzyme-blocks, protein derangements or cytopathogenetics. in addition to mere advice, he is able to thrust back further the barriers of uncertainty and to detect heterozygotes and late onset cases . . . thus far from being an inactive advisor, the counselor, as i see it, converts to the status of active moderator." in conclusion, it is almost tautological to stress that mental disorder constitues a formidable challenge to the medical world and to society in general. psychiatry has lagged a little behind the rapid progress made in general medicine — having been snared in a network of classifications and bedevilled with nebulous concepts of "psyche-pathology". it is therefore all the more gratifying to see it caught up now on the giant tidal wave of human genetics — physiologic genetics — and elevated from the esoteric depths of conceptual thinking to the crest of eclectic, concrete formulation and application. summary. there appears to be a lack of appreciation of the problem of mental disorder on the part of both the medical profession and the layman. the ^urrent interest in medical genetics has seemed to many to be a somewhat narrow speciality. however, mental disorder and genetic research have in fact been mutually fructifying. only recently has the hereditary component in mental illness been accorded concrete status in the face of the former prevailing environmentalist doctrine. consideration is given to examples within the broader classifications of, (a) mental deficiency and (b) the psychoses. (a) simple oligophrenia is briefly alluded to against the polygenic background of intelligence. the discoveries in the cytogenetics of mongolian idiocy are discussed arid it is noted that though the key to etiogenesis is now known, the door to the related biochemistry and pathology remains closed. phenylketonuria is presented as the perfect example of an inborn error of metabolism. heterozygotes for the condition can be detected by means of blood tests and this constitutes a step forward in terms of eugenic counseling. treatment with a low phenylalanine diet at birth offers chances of a happy outcome. in view of the success of therapy, and ease of diagnosis, a plea is made for the routine testing of all neo-nates (as practised overseas). (b) of the psychoses, manic depressive psychosis and schizophrenia are shown to be genetically determined. furthermore, the role of the neurohormones, acetylcholine, nor-adrenalin, adrenalin and serotonin in brain physiology is mentioned. disturbance at the genetic level leads to disturbance in the chemical homeostasis of the aforementioned substances, manifesting ultimately in mental aberration. many of the drugs used in the treatment of these endogenous pyschoses have furnished further indictment of the part played by the neurohormones in the genesis of mental illness. with entry into the realm of genes, molecules and atoms, our understanding of the psychoses is placed on a fundamental and firm footing. a genetic counseling unit has been established at the johannesburg general hospital under the aegis of professor l. a. hurst. various problems of a medico-genetic nature are dealt with, not least of all, those in the field of mental disorder. it is a happy augury that psychiatry, which has somewhat lagged behind the rapid progress made in general medicine, has attained a clear scientific approach with the incorporation of the basic science of genetics into its discipline. opsomming. dit lyk asof beide die mediese professie en die leek nie die erns van geestesgebreke insien nie. die huidige belangstelling in mediese genetika word deur meer as een as 'n baie beperkte rigting van spesialisasie beskou. geestegebreke en genetiese navorsing het egter, gesamentlik baie vrugte afdecember, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 3 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) gewerp. dit was maar onlangs wat die rol wat oorerwing speel in geestesgebreke sy ware status bereik het, in vergelyking met omgewingsleer. spesiale aandag word geskenk aan voorbeelde binne die wyer klasifikasie van, (a) swaksinnigheid (b) sielsiekte. (a) daar word kortliks verwys na eenvoudige oligofrenie, gesien teen die poligeniese agtergrond van intelligensie. uit die bespreking van die ontdekkings in die sitogenetika van mongoolse idiootheid is bemerk dat die oorerflikheids oorsake nou bekend is, maar die verwante biochemie en patologie is nog onbekend. fenielketonurie word voorgehou as die perfekte voorbeeld van 'n aangebore defek van metabolisme. heterosigote vir die toestand kan opgespoor word deur middel van bloedtoetse en dit is reeds vordering in eugeniese raadgewing. behandeling met 'n lae fenielalanien dieet met geboorte dui op 'n goeie prognose. na aanluiding van die sukses. van terapie en die vergemakliking van diagnose, word 'n beroep gedoen vir die roetine toetsing van alle pasgebore babas (soos oorsee gedoen word). (b) van die sielsiektes word die maniesdepressiewe psigose en die gesplete persoonlikheid geneties bepaal. verder word die rol van neurohormone, asetielkolien, nor-adrenalien, adrenalien en serotonien in brein-fisiologie genoem. steuring op die genetiese vlak lei tot steurings in die chemiese homeostase van die bogenoemde stowwe, wat uiteindelik geopenbaar word in geestesafwykings. baie van die verdowingsmiddels, wat gebruik word vir die behandeling van hierdie psigoses, het verdere aanduiding verskaf van die rol wat neurohormone, in die veroorsaking van sielsiektes, speel. met die toetrede tot die gebied van genes, molekules en atome, het ons begrip 'n vaste grondlegging gekry. 'n genetiese raadgewingseenheid is in die lewe geroep by die johannesburg algemene hospitaal onder die beskerming van prof. l. a. hurst. verskei probleme, van mediesgenetiese aard, word ondersoek; waaronder ook probleme op die gebied van geestesgebreke. dit is verblydend dat psigiatrie, wat agtergeraak het by die snelle vooruitgang van algemene medisyne, 'n suiwer wetenskaplike benadering geniet, met die insluiting van die basiese wetenskap van genetika. references. 1. opening vistas in psychiatry. l.a. hurst. witwatersrand university press, 1959. 2. applications of genetics in psychiatry and neurology. l. a. hurst. s.a. jnl. of lab. and clin. med. 1958. 4/3. p.169. 3. converging advances in psychiatric genetics and the pharmacology of psychotropic drugs. l. a. hurst. medical proceedings 1961. 7/20. p. 417. 4. the genetics of mental deficiency. i. f. anderson. the leech. oct. 1962. 5. genetic prognosis. i. f. anderson. 2nd. congress of s.a. genetics society, pretoria. 1st october, 1962 (in the press). 6. phenylketonuria in a mentally defective population. i. f. anderson, (in the press). 7. the strategy of the genes. c. h. waddington. george allen and unwin. ltd., london. 1957. journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an investigation into the language and generalization abilities in the brain-injured and non brain-injured mentally retarded child ilana o k e n introduction and aims j. l. khanna in his book "brain damage and mental retardation" reports a study by mednick and wild in which they found that the brain-injured child has a diminished degree of generalization responsiveness. (1968, 9.) it would seem that if a child has difficulty with generalization, he should also have difficulty in language development where a word must come to stand for an action or an object in many different situations and contexts. one of the most salient features of the mentally retarded child is his impoverished verbal behaviour, i.e. his delayed and impaired language. (1964, 18.) luria regards the basic symptom of the mentally retarded child, or the oligophrenic child as he terms it, as a profound disturbance in the operations of abstraction and generalization which manifests itself in, and dominates the whole of cognitive functioning. (1963, 11.) in terms of the above the writer felt that it would be interesting to conduct a comparative study of two groups of mentally retarded children, one of them brain-injured, the other non brain-injured, to see if there is any relationship between the differences in their language and generalization abilities, for if there is, this could be of great therapeutic and prognostic value. goldstein has described the brain-damaged adult as being deficient in the use of the "abstract attitude". perhaps this, as well as the concept of "rigidity" in the brain-injured child's thinking as postulated by goldstein, werner and others (1954, 13) (1966, 6) can be viewed as a diminished generalization ability, and these ideas have led the writer to investigate this field in the brain-injured child. most of the research on the language of the retardedis confined primarily to the study of speech. the research that is^cited on the "language" of mentally retarded children tends to reveal only that these children are defective on various measures of language without actually describing retarded children's language pattern. in using the illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities, (the itpa), the writer attempted to describe various areas of strengths and weaknesses within the two samples of retarded children, investigating: (i) whether the frequently stated assumption that the brain-injured child has a "peaked profile" and that the mentally retarded child has journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the brain-injured and n o n brain-injured mentally retarded child 33 a "relatively flat profile" exists, and whether this differentiation exists in the language abilities of the two diagnostic groups; (ii) whether retarded children differ psycholinguistically from average children of comparable mental age; (iii) whether there is a "typical" profile for the brain-injured mentally retarded and the non brain-injured mentally retarded. the writer was unable to find any test of generalization which could be used (other than complex equipment involving machinery) and, therefore, thought that it would be advantageous to devise a simple test which could be used diagnostically, therapeutically and prognostically. the writer views generalization as the grouping together of stimuli or objects which are similar, but not identical, to form a class, a category or a concept. parsins, skill et al state that this process is "the categorization of the particular, concrete objects of his situation into classes". (1955, 20.) hubert alexander clearly differentiated abstraction from generalization. he says that these processes are the reverse of each other, for generalization is the process of grouping like units together into classes, whereas abstracting is a process of selecting out attributes by focusing upon them. (1967, 1.) however, the writer feels that generalization involves abstracting to the extent that objects or events are grouped together by virtue of the certain abstractable traits which they have in common. rosenstein feels that concept formation is a progression from perception, to abstraction to generalization. he feels that concept formation is dependent on abstraction, and that concept utilization on generalization. (1961, 15.) (1963, 16.) strauss and kephart regard generalization as being essential in the development of concepts. they feel that generalization first occurs on a perceptual level, where the child makes perceptual responses to his object world by classifying, e.g. four cornered figures are classified as "squares" irrespective of their size, colour, whether they are solid figures or outline shapes. this is generalization of shape. as the child develops, a transition occurs from the perceptual generalizations to generalization over a less immediate or direct route. categorizations are now made according to function, use, location and later according to higher level groupings. (1955, 20.) the basic theme of the book "a study of thinking" by bruner, goodnow and austin is that virtually all cognitive activity is dependent on the process of categorizing. (1957, 5.) bruner developed this theme further by emphasizing that the development of a language system is a prerequisite to making generalizations beyond immediate environmental situations. (1966, 8.) rosenstein also feels that the development of concepts is greatly aided by language and other symbols. (1963, 16.) many learning theorists have differentiated between primary and secondary stimulus generalization. irv bialer defines primary stimulus generalization as "a response to superficial sensory similarity of stimuli which have been given otherwise distinctive characteristics in the form tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 liana oken of different labels". he says that in secondary stimulus generalization the utilization of learned equivalence is required. (1961, 4.) leventhal says that categorization is assumed to be on a perceptual level in the case of primary stimulus generalization and on a conceptual lever in the case of secondary stimulus generalization. he distinguishes between perception and conception in terms of: 1. speed and locus of action. perception operates quickly and· refers to objects present in the environment. conception operates more slowly on the basis of symbolic, non-sensory bound cues. 2. susceptibility to the influence of irrelevant and redundant information. perception is more greatly influenced by these than is conception. vygotsky views the formation of concepts as a developmental sequence of generalizations, at first grouping syncretic heaps together, i.e. with no apparent principle in mind, then progressing to a higher level of generalization where he groups according to unique associations, e.g. toys "belong" in his playpen, he then proceeds to group according to some functional relationship, and the epitome of categorization in vygotsky's system occurs with the mastering of the superordinate. (1962, 22.) (1967, 19.) experimental methodology 1. subjects: ten subjects were used in this study. five of these children formed, the brain-injured group, and the other five the non brain-injured group. all the subjects were mentally retarded and enrolled in special classes in government schools in johannesburg. for the purpose of classifying the subjects into the two experimental groups, the writer used a combination of the riggs and rain classification system (1952, 14) and the system proposed by schulman, kaspar and throne. (1965, 17.) the criteria for selection into the "brain-injured" group are some of those laid down for the riggs and rain "organic" group, and the "non brain-injured" group are equivalent to schulman et al group 2, i.e. "patients at the low end of the normal distribution curve". four out of the five subjects in the non brain-injured group fit into the riggs and rain group of "unexplained", i.e. children who, in spite of fairly detailed case histories, can be called neither familial nor organic without resorting to inference rather than to fact. the fifth child's retardation is of the familial type. / (a) brain-injured group: the subjects used in this group were selected in terms of meeting with one of the following criteria: (i) diagnosis of brain-injury by a neurologist; (ii) case history indicating anoxia at birth; (iii) encephalitis before baby aged six months; (iv) epilepsy diagnosed medically; three of the subjects had been 'diagnosed as brain-injured by neurologists. b5 had hemiparesis on the right side, b4 had frequent grand journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e brain-injured and n o n brain-injured mentally retarded child ' 35 mal epileptic attacks and b2 had nystagmus in the right eye. the case histories of b1 and b3 indicate anoxia at birth. b4 had encephalitis at two weeks, which was followed by the epileptic attacks, and b5 was the second of a stillborn twin, weighed \ \ pounds at birth, suffered from anoxia at birth and was given a transfusion immediately after birth. (b) non brain-injured group: the children in this group were selected in terms of the following criteria: (i) no organic cause present in the case history; (ii) no organic symptomatology present; (iii) these children were diagnosed as mentally retarded on the basis of an intelligence test. (c) intelligence: before a child is admitted to a special class in a government school, he has to be assessed by the school psychologist. the child's intelligence is tested on the south african individual scale. thus the intelligence of all the children was tested on the same scale, and all the tests were done after january 1968. the iq scores of the subjects ranged from 65 to 80. (d) socio-economic class: four of the subjects were at gresswold school and the other six were in the "coaching class" at emmarentia primary school. both schools are in the northern suburbs of johannesburg, and all the children were of middle-class parents. none of the children were, or had ever been institutionalized; all children lived at home. (e) chronological age: the chronological ages of the children ranged between 7 and 11 years. because of difficulties in getting children of the same chronological age (ca) and iq, the subjects were matched in pairs as closely as possible with respect to ca, iq and sex where possible. (f) further criteria for selection were: (i) the subjects had to be ambulatory and have sufficient motor coordination to carry out the tasks, e.g. pointing, picking up blocks; (ii) children with severe personality and emotional problems were not used; however, some of the children in the brain-injured group did show some of the "strauss-type" behaviour symptoms, but none of these problems was severe; (iii) the children had to be able to express their basic needs and understand simple directions; (iv) the children had to be able to differentiate shapes of a triangle, circle and square. they had to be able to post the appropriate shape in the "post box" task; (v) the teachers of the children reported that none of the childrenhad difficulty with colour perception, and none had any hearing difficulty. 2. procedure: (a) conditions for the experiment: the environment in which the experiment was carried out was familiar to all the children. eight tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 liana oken of the children were tested at their schools, and they were taken out of their regular class one at a time and were tested in a classroom at the far end of the school. the situation was controlled as far as possible for both extraneous auditory and visual stimuli. the two children not tested at school were tested at their homes, at a table in a quiet room. (b) the tests administered were: i. the itpa. ii. a generalization test devised by the writer which was administered following a pause of five minutes after the completion of the itpa. (c) standardization of the generalization test: the generalization test was given to 12 "normal" children aged between and 7 years. these 12 children formed the standardization group. they lived in either gresswold or emmarentia, and were, therefore, children of the same socio-economic class as the subjects in the experimental groups. the purpose of giving this test to the normal children was to roughly standardize the test, to get an idea as to how normal children would perform on this test so as to be able to compare the performance of the experimental groups with the normal group. 3. materials: i. the itpa: this is a diagnostic test of language abilities. it is a battery of nine subtests, each subtest assessing a particular aspect of the global area of language ability. the itpa was administered in the recommended order and in accordance with the instructions laid out in the manual. (1961, 12.) the only exception was a five-minute pause after the visual-motor sequencing subtest. this pause was given to all subjects, as the writer felt that the test was a long one, and that the pause would overcome, to a certain extent, the element of fatigue. ii. the generalization test: (a) theoretical framework on which the generalization test is based: generalization or categorization, as has been shown by several authorities (strauss and kephart, rosenstein, vygotsky and others) is a developmental phenomenon, and proceeds from the concrete perceptual to the more complex conceptual mode. the child at first operates on the basis of superficial, sensory bound cues and groups according to what he "sees" and what "is". he then proceeds to group according to more conceptual aspects, not to what "is" iri the present, but according to less stimulus bound and more symbolic cues. he now begins to group dissimilar looking cues into one group and regards them as being "the same" in terms of their functions, uses, locations, etc. the test is, therefore, divided! into three levels, all increasing in complexity. there are three pretest items, the purpose of which is to illustrate to the child that he must group items together that constitute one category, e.g. "squares". journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e brain-injured and n o n brain-injured mentally retarded child 37 section 1 of the test involves grouping together of blocks of different sizes, shapes and, later, colours into particular categories. here the child is required to generalize in terms of the objects he sees before him—this is on a concrete perceptual level. section 2 of the test involves the child choosing one picture out of three that can be grouped with three other pictures, all of one category or stimulus class. here the child is required to form the generalization in terms of the picture that he sees. section 3 requires the child to select one word out of four that does not fit into the category. here the child is required to generalize three of the words into a category or higher level concept, and verbalize the word that does not fit into the desired category. each item of every subtest is graded in difficulty, and thus it is expected that a child who fails item (d) in the first section of the test will not pass items in the second section of the test. (b) description of the generalization test: pretest: in this part of the test, nine blocks are used, three identical squares, three identical triangles and three identical circles. they are identical with respect to colour, size and shape. the three squares and the circle are placed in front of the child and the child is instructed to place all the blocks that are "the same" into the tester's hand. if the child succesfully completes item (a) by himself, the same procedure is carried out for pretest items (b) and (c). if the child fails item (a), the tester shows the child how to succesfully complete the task, and explains to the child that the three squares are the same because they all look alike, all have four sides and all are "squares". the child is then given item (b) to do. if the child fails item (b), he is again shown how to successfully complete the task, and the tester explains the reasons for the grouping. the child is then given item (c). should he fail this item, the test is then stopped. w = white tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 l i a n a oken section 1: in this section, blocks of various sizes, shapes and colours are used, and the child has to group those objects that are the "same". sameness of shape is the type of grouping required. the five items are: blocks for'item (a) are randomly placed on the table before the child. the child is again instructed to place those blocks that are "the same" into the tester's hand. if the child is able to do this without assistance, the test is continued in the same manner. if the child is unable to do the item, or groups according to colour, the tester shows the child the correct way of grouping. the child is' then given item (b) to do; if the child fails to do this correctly, the tester again shows and explains the correct grouping; if the child then fails item (c), the test is stopped. section 2: in this part of the test, white cards 2\ inches/by 3} inches with clear pictures pasted on them are used. the child is required to group pictures together that are of the same category. the following categories are used: (a) animals; '(b) furniture; (c) people; (d) parts of the face; (e) jewellery. before commencing the test, the tester explains to the child which is the "top row" and which is the "bottom row". the child is then required to indicate which row is which on command. the child is then instructed to show the tester which card on the bottom row is "the same as, the same thing as, goes with" the journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 (c) r r = red β = blue y = yellow r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e brain-injured and n o n brain-injured mentally retarded child 39 three cards in the top row. the instructions are given to the child in these three ways to ensure that he understands what is required of him. if the child completes the item successfully, the test is continued in the same manner. if the child fails item (a) the tester shows the child the correct way of grouping, explaining that they are all of one category, e.g. animals, and the child is then given item (b) to do, and the same procedure is followed as that in section 1. the cards used in this section of the test are placed on the table in the following order: (a) 1. elephant 2. cat 3. horse 4. dog 5. boy 6. car (b) 1. cupboard 2. chair 3. table 4. dress 5. bed 6. flower (c) 1. mother 2. boy 3. father 4. dog 5. girl 6. table (d) 1. eyes 2. ears 3. mouth 4. nose 5. apple 6. dress (e) 1. brooch 2. watch 3. necklace 4. chair 5. girl 6. ring section 3: this section has an example given to the ch: explain what is required of him, and five items then follow. in the example item the words "dog, cow, ball, pig" are given and, if the child responds correctly, the test is continued as in sections 1 and 2. if the child fails to give the correct response, the tester explains to the child why "ball" is the word that does not fit. the child is then given item (a) to do and, if he fails, he is given the correct response and an explanation as to the reason. if the child then fails item (b), the test is stopped. the series of words used in this section of the test are: (a) hat boat shoes coat (b) car train ship mountain (c) fork banana apple grape (d) river sea sand swimming-pool (e) lamp light ball sun (c) general points in the administration of the test: 1. after three successive failures in any part of the test the test was stopped. 2. after each failure the child was shown the correct response. 3. when the child performed correctly, the tester acknowledged this by saying "good, that's right". 4. after each item in all sections of the test, the tester asked the child why he had grouped the objects, pictures or words in the way that he had, and the child's verbal responses were recorded. (d) scoring of the generalization test: every item of the test was weighted with a score one more than the score preceding it. thus passing item (a) yielded a score of 1, passing item (b) yielded a score 1 + 2, i.e. 3, passing item (c) yielded tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr.: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 liana oken a score of 6, i.e. 1 + 2 + 3. the total number of points in the test was 120. results 1. the itpa: t a h i j : i: m e a n itpa proeile and r a n g e s or b r a i n i n j u r e d m e n t a l l y retarded groui' + 3 00' representational level anioin-sc.qnential + 3 00' decoding association encoding autom. sequential + 3 00' 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 + 3 00' and. visual and. vocal visual motor vocal motor and. vocal and. vocal visual motor + 3 00' + 2-50 + 2 0 0 + 150 + 1 00 + -50 00 -50 — 1 00 / s . 1 5 0 / 1/ s \ 2 00 f s 2 5 0 3 0 0 the total language age scores of the two groups did not yield significant differences between the two groups, yet as can be seen from the above profiles the two diagnostic groups performances differ. both groups have "peaked profiles" but these profiles are peaked in different areas, i.e. the groups show different strengths and weaknesses on the various subtests. the brain-injured groups performance on visual subtests was inferior to its performance on auditory subtests on all levels. when this result was subjected to statistical analysis it was found to be significant at the ,001 level of confidence. the non brain-injured groups performance was poorer in the auditory channel for all subtests except auditory-vocal journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the brain-injured and n o n brain-injured mentally retarded child 41 t a b l e 2: m e a n itpa profile and r a n g e s of n o n b r a i n i n j u r e d m e n t a l l y retarded g r o u p . + 3-00 representational level antom-seqnential + 3-00 decoding association encoding an torn. sequential + 3-00 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 + 3-00 and. visual and. vocal visual motor vocal motor and. vocal and. vocal visual motor + 3-00 + 2-50 -f 2 · 00 -!-1 · 50 -h 1 -00 + -50 00 -50 1 0 0 — 1-50 \ \ -2-00 \ >ν -2-50 -3-00 sequencing. the result of this score was influenced by the extreme range in this subtest due to the heterogeneity in performance of the subjects in the group on this test. when the students t test was applied to this result, the difference was not found to be statistically significant. the non brain-injured group were statistically inferior to the braininjured group on the auditory-vocal automatic subtest at the ,02 level of significance. the brain-injured group was inferior to the non brain-injured group in the whole decoding process, and thus although the non brain-injured group were poorer in their auditory as compared with their visual subtests, their performance on auditory decoding was superior to that of the brain-injured group. a t test was performed to see whether the difference between the groups performances on decoding and encoding were significant or not. the difference between the brain-injured groups encoding performance was not statistically significant, the non braininjured group were statistically superior to the brain-injured group in the decoding process at the ,001 level of confidence. in both groups motor encoding was superior to vocal encoding. both groups performed extremely poorly on both association subtests, the tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. : des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 liana oken brain-injured group were more inferior due to their deficit in the visual channel. the range for each subtest was calculated. this is the difference between the uppermost and lowest scores on a particular subtest. it is interesting to observe the size of the ranges on the particular subtests. mostof the subtests show fairly large ranges indicative of heterogenous performances by the subjects in the groups, however, both the association subtests have relatively small ranges indicating that all of the children in the two groups performed poorly on these subtests. it seems as though in this sample of brain-injured and non brain-injured mentally retarded children, all the children had difficulty in this area irrespective of their individual differences. within each diagnostic group a t test was performed to determine whether the groups abilities in encoding and decoding differed significantly. the difference between the brain-injured group's decoding and encoding abilities was significant at the ,001 level of confidence, and the difference between the non brain-injured group's decoding and encoding abilities was significant at the ,01 level. when the mental ages of the subjects in both groups were compared with their language ages, it was found that these scores do not correlate well, but that there was a considerable difference between the two. the difference was found to be significant at the ,001 level of confidence. ii. results on generalization test: (a) standardization group: the following general trends in performance were observed: (i) children between and 4 \ were able to complete all perceptual groupings and up to (d) of section 2., thus their scores were approximately 45. (ii) children between 4j and were able to complete all perceptual and concrete conceptual groupings. they were able to complete up to section 3's (d), thus their scores were approximately 91. (iii) children over 5j were able to complete the whole test successfully. thus their scores were 120. (b) experimental groups: t a b l e 3 : r e s u l t s o n g e n e r a l i z a t i o n t e s t o f t h e b r a i n i n j u r e d a n d n o n b r a i n i n j u r e d g r o u p s brain-injured non brain-injured 1. 106 47 2. 15 105 3. 98 58 • 4. 37 0 5. 14 36 total: 270 246 m e a n : 54 49-2 journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e brain-injured and n o n brain-injured mentally retarded child 43 the brain-injured groups performance on this test was superior to the performance of the non brain-injured group, but when a t test was performed on the above data it showed that the difference was not statistically significant. the reasons for the lack of significant results could be due to the following: 1. small size of groups, and large range of possible scores. 2. heterogeneity in performance of subjects closely matched for ca and iq. the subjects performance on this test was compared to the performance of normalchildren in the standardization group. the generalization ability of both the brain-injured and non brain-injured groups appeared to be far inferior to their mental ages. discussion from the findings of the present study one cannot emphatically state that there is a "typical" profile for the diagnostic groups, but it does seem that there are certain trends in performance of the two groups. in the gallagher study it was reported that there were tendencies for the brain-injured group to be superior to the non brain-injured group on items tapping the visual channel. (1957, 7.) in the present study the brain-injured group were significantly inferior in the visual channel for each process, but the deficit of the non brain-injured group in the auditory channel, though evident was not statistically significant. strauss et al regard perceptual deficits as being characteristic of the brain-injured child. they say that "brain-injured children show a disintegration in the visual-perceptual field" (1947, 21.), and they use as their diagnostic tests of "brain-injury" tests of visual perception, e.g. the werner-strauss marble board. bateman reports that children who show a visual motor disability on the itpa are the children with "perceptual disorganization" or the "strauss-syndrome" children. (1965, 2.) the deficit in the automatic sequential level which has been reported in the literature on the itpa with mentally retarded children was evident in the non brain-injured group. their performance was poor over the entire automatic-sequential level, but they showed the greatest deficit on this level in the auditory-vocal automatic subtest. the writer wishes to postulate that the poor performance on this subtest is related to their deficient generalization ability indicated on the generalization test. when children learn the grammar of their language they are not formally taught these rules. they hear different grammatical structures in their environment and from this they formulate a rule which they have to generalize in order to produce novel utterances. however they do not hear a rule in one way, but in a number of ways, i.e. in a number of different utterances all obeying that rule, and they have to automatically generalize the rule which they have learnt in situation a to situation b. therefore a child who is deficient in his generalization ability will be deficient in a subtest tapping his ability to learn grammatical rules automatically. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 l i a n a oken the greatest deficit in both diagnostic groups was in the association process. bateman and wetherell report that the type of association process assessed by the itpa can be considered either as a retrieval or organization and generalization stage. they feel that it is impossible to determine on an auditory-vocal association deficit alone whether the problem is one of retrieval or organization and generalization. however they feel that the visual-motor association subtest is more heavily weighted with organization. (1965, 3.) since both groups in this study performed poorly on both the association subtests, the writer feels that these two tests are both tapping generalization ability. in the performance of the subjects on the generalization test it was interesting to observe the manner in which the children of the different groups made their generalizations. some of the responses of the braininjured children were bizarre and unrelated to the stimulus presentations particularly in section 2 of the test. in this section some of the braininjured children tended to make their groupings according to an association between one of the pictures in the bottom row and one of the pictures in the top row, e.g. boy and horse cause "boy sits on a horse". these associations tended to be on the basis of contiguity as in the above example, or activity between the two objects, e.g. boy and horse cause they both "go". an interesting observation was the clear difference between grouping or generalizing on the perceptual and conceptual levels. subjects b3, b4 and b5 experienced no difficulty in section 1, yet were unable to do the items in section 2. the performance of these subjects on the association subtest was their most inferior performance. subject m4 had no difficulty in doing the pretest items, yet was unable to group stimuli differing in one respect, i.e. size. the subject showed the poorest generalization ability on the generalization test, and his itpa scores showed the poorest deficits on auditory-vocal and auditory-vocal automatic subtests. an important factor in generalization ability seemed to be the verbalization accompanying the grouping. the most successful subjects were those who were able to give the name of the category they were grouping, and it seemed as though appropriate use of verbal activity was the important factor, yet the subjects who merely named the picture they selected, e.g. "cause it's a dog" were more successful than those subjects who made no verbalizations. this point seems to illustrate luria's thesis of speech regulating behaviour and also strauss and kephart's idea that the use of the category word unifies perceptions and later conceptions. the findings of this study support the use of the itpa as a diagnostic tool. the writer does not feel that a diagnosis can be made from the results on this test alone, but analysis of the profile can point in the direction of a particular diagnosis.! knowledge of etiology seems to be important as it indicates the type of therapy programme to be used. if the child is found to be brain-injured, the therapy or remediation journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e brain-injured and n o n brain-injured mentally retarded child 45 prograifime should stress the auditory avenue, i.e. an auditory approach to language training, as the stronger avenue can be used to facilitate the weaker. bateman and wetherell feel that an important implication in the education of retarded children can be derived from the knowledge of a deficit in automatic rote aspects of language usage. the need for repetition, over learning and drills seems obvious, yet by making all learning situations meaningful to the child the retarded child will handle these tasks exclusively at the representational level, thus further strengthening his already relatively strong representational skills and neglecting the automatic-sequential abilities which are in need of exercise. (1965, 3.) because the performance of both groups of children is deficient in the association process of the itpa, therapy and education for the retarded child should place a direct emphasis on the formation of concepts, abstractions and generalizations. the generalization test as it stands can be used prognostically, as, the child who performs superiorly on it should be more successful in therapy and achieve more in a shorter period of time than a child who shows poor generalization ability, as he will form concepts and thus learn language quicker. the generalization test as it stands or modifications of it can be used therapeutically to train children in generalization. the performance of the child on the generalization test can also be a valuable therapeutic guide indicating from what point to begin therapy, whether one need work on a very concrete perceptual level or on a more conceptual level. conclusions the total language age scores of the two groups did not yield significant differences between the two groups. however, when the performance of the two groups were broken down and analysed significant differences in areas of strengths and weaknesses were observed. both groups performed most inferiorly on the association subtests of the itpa, and this performance correlated well with the poor performance of the subjects on the generalization test. the writer feels that there is a close inter-relationship between language ability and generalization ability, and this illustrates the importance of giving the child certain forms of non-language or perceptual training before beginning language therapy as it has been shown that these pre-language training procedures form the basis for conceptual development. summary five brain-injured children were compared with five non brain-injured children on the itpa and a generalization test devised by the writer. these children were aged between 7 and 11 years, and their iq scores tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 l i a n a oken were between 65 and 80. all the children were in special classes in government schools in johannesburg, and were free of auditory, visual and physical disabilities. the results of this investigation were presented and discussed in terms of whether any difference in language ability of the subjects in this study was present, so as to see whether there was a "typical" profile for each diagnostic group. it was concluded that there are different types of peaks in the profiles of the two groups, and that both groups have poor generalization ability. a close inter-relationship' between language and generalization abilities was considered to exist. opsomming vyf kinders met breinbesering is vergelyk met vyf kinders sonder breinbesering deur middel van die itpa en 'n veralgemeningstoets wat deur die skrywer opgestel is. die kinders se ouderdomme was tussen 7 en 11 jaar, en hulle ik's tussen 65 en 80. alle kinders woon spesiale klasse in staatskole in johannesburg by, en was sonder gehoor, gesigs en fisiese gebreke. die resultate van hierdie ondersoek was bespreek in terme van die moontlike verskil in taalvermoe van die twee groepe. daar was gepoog om te sien of daar wel 'n profiele was wat „tiperend" is van elke diagnostiese groep. die afleiding is gemaak dat daar verskillende soorte pieke in die profiele van die twee groepe voorkom, en beide groepe het 'n swak veralgemeningsvermoe. dit het geblyk dat daar 'n noue verband tussen taal en veralgemeningsvermoe was. references 1. alexander, h. g. (1967): language and thinking. d. von nostrand co., inc., new york. 2. bateman, b. (1965): "the role of the illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities in differential diagnosis and programme planning." american 'journal of orthopsychiatry, 35, pp. 465-472. 3. bateman, b. and wetherell, j. (1965): "psycholinguistic aspects of mental retardation." mental retardation, 3, 2, pp. 8-14. 4. bialer, 1. (1961): "primary and secondary stimulus generalization as related to intelligence level." journal of experimental psychology, 62, 4, . pp. 395-402. 5. bruner, j. s., goodnow, j. j. and austin, g. a. (1957): a study of thinking. john wiley & sons, inc., new york. 6. copelowitz, l. b. (1966): "perceptual and conceptual defects of the braini n j u r e d child (including the cerebral palsy)." unpublished dissertation for the degree of b.a. logopedics, university of the witwatersrand. 7. gallagher, j. j. (1957): "a comparison of brain-injured and n o n braininjured mentally retarded children on several psychological variables." monographs of the society for research in child development, 22, 2, n o . 8. hamilton, j. (1966): "learning of a generalized response class in mentally retarded individuals." american journal of mental deficiency, 71, i, pp. 1 0 0 1 0 8 . • 9. k h a n n a , j. l. (1968): brain damage and mental retardation. charles c. thomas. springfield, illinois, u.s.a.| 10. leventhal, d. s. (1966): "a study of the roles of perceptual and conceptual categorization." dissertation abstracts, 27, 118, pp. 316-317. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the brain-injured and n o n brain-injured mentally retarded child 47 11. luria, a. r. (1963): the mentally retarded child. p e r g a m o n press, new york. 12. mccarthy, j. j. and kirk, s.a. (1961): illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities—examiner's manual. university of illinois, institute for research on exceptional children, urbana illin. 13. mcmurry, j. g. (1954): "rigidity in conceptual thinking in exogenous and endogenous mentally retarded children." journal of consulting psychology, 18, 5, pp: 366-370. 14. riggs, μ. m. and rain, μ. e. (1952): "a classification system for the mentally retarded." the training school bulletin. 49, pp. 75-84. 15. rosenstein, j. (1961): "perception, cognition and language in deaf children." exceptional children. .27, pp. 276-284. 16. rosenstein, j. (1963): "concept development and language instruction." exceptional children. 30, pp. 337-343. 17. schulman, j. l., kaspar, j. c. and throne, f . m . (1965): brain damage and behaviour. a clinical experimental study. charles c. thomas. springfield. illinois. 18. siegal, g. m. (1964): "prevailing concepts in speech research with mentally retarded children." asha, 6, pp. 192-194. 19. silverman, t. r. (1967): "categorisation behaviour and achievement in deaf and hearing children." exceptional children. 34, 4, pp. 241-250. 20. strauss, a. a. and kephart, n. c. (1955) :psychqpathology and education of the brain-injured child. g r u n e & stratton, new york. 21. strauss, a. a. and lehtinen, l. (1947): psycho pathology and education of the brain-injured child. g r u n e & stratton. new york. 22. vygotsky, l. s. (1962): thought and language. m.i.t. press. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a preliminary assessment of radical surgery in cleft palate deformities d a v i e s , d a v i d , f.r.c.s.* whiting, d.m., b.a. log. ( r a n d ) , l.c.s.t. miller, b.h., b.d.s. ( r a n d ) , d . orth. f.d.s. r.c.s. (eng.) cremin, b.j., m.b.b.s., m.r.c.s., m.c.r.a., f.f.r. morrison, g., f.c.s. (s.a.) *plastic surgery, groote schuur hospital, university of cape town summary the d e v e l o p m e n t and aims of the single stage repair of c o m p l e t e clefts of the lip and palate are presented together with a preliminary account of results. opsomming die ontwikkeling en die doelstellings van 'n enkele stadium herstel van gesplete lip en verhemelte word aangebied saam met voorafgaande bepalinge van die resultate. in the repair of cleft lip and palate deformities surgeons have always considered these two entities as separate deformities when corrective surgery was undertaken. indeed many authorities 5 · 6 · 1 1 · 1 2 · 1 3 today, still prefer to repair the lip alone at any age from 48 hours after birth to 3 months; followed later by correction of the hard palate deformity, and at the age of one year to eighteen months, a final operation to close the soft palate defect. some indeed even regret the fact that the two deformities are associated. consider this quotation from brown and mcdowell1; it is unfortunate that cleft lips so frequently coexist with cleft palate causing many surgeons to be preoccupied with closing part of the palate at the same time the lip is closed. aside from the probability that early surgical treatment to the palate may result in unnecessary dental damage, it seems to us that good repair of the lip is difficult enough to require the surgeons undivided attention in the process. perusing the literature we can see that there are those who follow the conventional pattern described above of lip repair hard palate repair soft palate repair. veau1 4, who did much to stimulate modern cleft palate surgery, repaired lip and hard palate together, followed later by repair of the soft palate. many followed this technique and schmid8, nordin and johanson7, schrudde and stellmach9, combined this with an immediate autogenous bone graft to close the alveolor gap. others such as slaughter11, schewendiek10 and longacre3, feel that the lip should be closed early, and at six months the soft palate should be repaired leaving the hard palate unrepaired until the age tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 19 desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 of 6-8 years. in this interval an obturator is used to fill the defect in the hard palate. the rationale is that early closure of the soft palate aids early and normal speech function while the untouched hard palate allows normal development of the maxillary arches, thus leading to less bony deformity and orthodontic disability. at the red cross war memorial children's hospital in cape town, our predecessors dr. norman peterson and dr. d.s. davies, followed the early pattern of lip hard palate soft palate closure, and later changed it to lip and hard palate soft palate closure. in 1964, we felt that a unilateral cleft lip and palate deformity should be treated as one congenital deformity and repaired at one operation. our rationale for this was firstly technical; all the structures are easily accessible and the operation is in many ways easier than the multistage procedures. secondly, the operation was completed in one stage which held many advantages for mother and child,·especially as a large number of our patients came from far flung country districts, and under poor socio-economic conditions it is always easier for the mother to care for these infants if the palate has been reconstructed at an early age. thirdly, we felt that the orthodontic aspect of this congenital deformity had been stressed at the expense of speech. normal speech, after all, should be the prime objective of all surgeons working in this field, and if this more extensive operation held forth the promise of improved speech results because of early complete closure, then it should be completely investigated. surgical procedures a series of operations was started2 and an effort was made to standardise the operation so that in our assessment of the speech and orthodontic results there should be as few variables as possible. we plan to complete 50 cases with a z-plasty lip repair, autogenous rib bone graft to alveolar gap, one layer vomerine mucosa repair of hard palate defect and a push back of the soft palate with a millard4 island flap. the next 50 cases will have z-plasty repair of the lip, no bone graft to alveolar gap, vomerine mucosa repair of the hard palate, and a 2 flap wardill16 push back repair of the soft palate with tvo millard island flap. finally 50 cases with z-plasty repair of lip and simple modern von langebeck15 repair of palate with no push back of the soft palate. no patients will be excluded from this series because of the size of the congenital defect, and there will be no particular selection of cases-t'o any of the above series. with this series we should be able to show whether early surgery of this magnitude is dangerous for the child or not; whether primary bone graft inhibits maxillary growth; whether early closure of the palate predisposes to more normal speech; whether early radical surgery leads to poorer orthodontic results and finally whether an island flap repair or a push back procedure has any advantages over a simple palatal closure. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a preliminary assessment of radical surgery in cleft palate deformities 5 we have so far completed 95 cases and it seems possible at this stage to say that the procedure is not technically difficult and there does not appear to be any added risk for the child. blood loss over the series has averaged 10% of" the blood volume, and the last twenty cases have lost only from 15 to 50 c.c. of blood, for a 2 hour procedure. obviously these operations must be done by a cohesive team and under the best anaesthetic conditions. it is clear from the above that we believe in early closure to achieve normal muscular action, at as early an age as possible. this principle should extend to simple clefts of the secondary palate, and bilateral complete clefts as well. ten of these have been closed as a one stage procedure. complete unilateral repairs have been done at 10 days of age, but in view of possible associated cardiac anomalies, which often manifest themselves only in the first month of life, we have now fixed our optimum age at 3 months. by this time the child should weigh more than 4,5 kgs. and have a normal haemoglobin level. obviously it is too early to draw any but the most sketchy and preliminary conclusions from this series. there is no place in any competent team for the plastic surgeon who attempts to assess his own results; these must be reviewed by a speech therapist and an orthodontist. speech assessment the format of the speech assessment used in our clinics is specifically designed for its clinical implications for future management of each individual case. velopharyngeal competence is judged objectively on tests and subjectively in spontaneous speech on a 4 point scale. mixed or deficient nasal resonance is noted when present. articulation is assessed in words first with the nose open and then all errors are checked with nostrils closed to determine whether the error is due to inadequate intra oral air pressure or incorrect placement of articulators. errors in tongue-tip consonants are checked with particular reference to irregularities in the anterior maxillary arch and upper dentition. a particular note is made as to whether articulation deteriorates in spontaneous speech or not. speech classification for cleft lip a n d palate a r e s o n a n c e a1 normal resonance. a2 slightly excessive nasal resonance. a3 medium nasal resonance. a4 gross nasal resonance. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 β a r t i c u l a t i o n b1 no articulation errors. b2 one or two articularion errors only. (nb phonetic equivalents such as s,z,; t,d; k,g,; i.e. voiced and voiceless counterparts are counted as 1 error.) b3 three or more articulation errors, but intelligible. (nb a patient with only 2 articulation errors in words, but whose articulation deteriorates in spontaneous speech is classified as b3.) b4 multiple articulation errors unintelligible. examples of: 1. s,z; r (2 errors) 2. s,z,; t,d (2 errors) 3. s , z „ sh, ge (2 errors) 1 . s,z,; sh, ge; ch,j. (3 errors) 2. s,z,; t,d„;r (3 errors) 3. s,z,; sh; ch; r (4 errors) b4 plosives as glottals, distortions or omissions of sibilants. c l a s s i f i c a t i o n e x a m p l e s αι b1 normal speech. αι b3 normal resonance, 3 or more articulation errors. a2 b2 slight nasal resonance, 1 or 2 articulation errors. a3 b1 medium nasal resonance, no articulation errors. a4 b4 gross nasal resonance. multiple articulation errors; unintelligible. to be classified as b2, a child's articulation errors remain the same in spontaneous speech as in words. the child who has only one or two articulation errors in words but whose articulation deteriorates in spontaneous speech is classified as b3. the results of the speech assessment together with results of x-ray, hearing evaluation, and case history will indicate whether the residual speech defect is due to:/ a) anatomical anomalies of 1) velopharyngeal mechanism or 2) anterior maxillary arch and dentition. b) incompetence of function c) other factors such as hearing loss, developmental lag, mental retardation, or cultural deprivation, etc. we include some preliminary figures from the one stage repair series, but it must be emphasised that this is an incomplete series, and we feel that some of these cases can be expected to improve as they grow up. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a preliminary assessment of radical surgery in cleft palate deformities unilateral cleft lip and palate one stage repair series a1 a2 a3 a4 complete assessment incomplete assessm ent total 11 11 4 1 27 19 4 6 b1 b2 b3 b4 complete assessment incomplete assessment total 6 9 9 3 27 19 4 6 table i analysis of speech results assessments between 1.1.70 and 30.4.72 20 cases age range — 4 yrs. 9 yrs. assessment between 1.1.68 and 31.12.70 7 cases age range — 4 yrs.-10 yrs. incomplete assessments — 19 cases in may '72 a1 a2 a3 a4 total 9 6 4 1 2 0 b1 b2 b3 b4 total 5 7 6 2 2 0 table ii analysis of complete assessments at follow-up between 1.1.71 30.4.72 orthodontic assessment no pre-surgical dental orthopaedics has been carried out on the maxillary arches of these patients. orthodontic assessment is made on:1. models pre-operative intra oral impressions of both upper and lower jaws are taken in the operation theatre immediately before the operation with the child anaesthetised. they are cast in stone and trimmed so that the correct relationship is shown between the upper and lower dental arches. these models are repeated usually at yearly intervals or at the appropriate time to show dimensional changes. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 19 desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 a1 a2 a3 a4 total group total + island flap 4 7 2 1 1 4 27 — island flap 7 4 2 13 27 b1 b2 b3 b4 total group total + island flap 3 3 6 2 14 27 — island flap 3 6 3 1 13 27 table iii speech results in relation to island flap in the 27 cases fully assessed. age b1 b2 b3 b4 total 4 2 1 3 2 8 5 3 4 2 9 6 1 1 2 7 1 1 2 8 1 1 2 9 1 2 3 10 1 1 total 6 9 9 3 27 table iv articulation and age at assessments of the 2 7 cases. 2. ' cephalometric x-rays / these are true lateral views of the skull and jaws taken with the head held in a fixed position, and at a standardised target film distance. they are therefore, strictly comparable to show growth changes in a longitudinal study. a sample series of 30 cases was used for a preliminary survey. in this instance only the pre-operative and most recently .taken dental models were used for assessment. the average age at operation was 6,7 months with a range from 4 months to two years. final models of the dental arches were taken at an average age of 3 years, the oldest being 5 years and 10 months. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a preliminary assessment of radical surgery in cleft palate deformities 9 b1 b2 • b3 β 4 total maxillary arch and dental a n o m a l i e s 0 5 3 8 dysfunction 0 2 1 3 other factors 0 2 3 • 5 undetermined 2 3 5 normal articulation 6 6 totals 6 9 9 3 η table v analysis of causes of articulation errors. results show that 47% of these patients have a class iii relationship of the incisor teeth, i.e. the lower incisors occluding in front of the upper. the remaining 53% have a class i (or normal) occlusion. the'maxillary minor segment showed varying degrees of collapse in 66,7% of cases. this was assessed by relating the teeth on the minor segment to the opposing mandibular teeth and noting the amount of collapse towards the midline. although the percentage of cases showing class iii incisor relationship and collapse of the minor segment may appear to be high, these figures compare quite favourably with those of other published series. also in many of these cases, simple orthodontic treatment is all that will be required to bring about correction. it will however, be necessary to follow up a larger number of cases over a longer period of time before any final conclusion can be reached regarding growth of the dento-facial complex. radiological assessment the radiological assessment of these cases consists of static and dynamic studies which are pre and post-operative. the static studies are cephalometric measurements which are performed in the first few months of life, using an infant cephalostat and-sedationv the dynamic studies are a joint examination done by a team which consists of at least the surgeon, radiologist, and speech therapist. the parents are also , often invited to attend. children from 3 years upwards are examined by a pulsed image intensifier using a 5 inch (magnification χ \ vz ) field projected " onto a television screen. the length and mobility of the soft palate, and its capacity for sustained naso-pharyngeal closure, during repeated set sentences, are noted and assessed. a simultaneous speech videotape recording is also tydskrf van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 made and interesting or difficult cases are immediately played back and restudied. the videotape can later be transferred by kinescope camera onto 16 mm. film for further study on an analector camera. the advantages of this method over 35mm. cine radiography are that it enables the team to make a definite and immediate assessment, as development and projection facilities are not required (except for the delayed studies). the irradiation exposure to the child, which can be considerable with cine radiography, is also greatly reduced. the soft tissues of the pharynx are so well visualised in these lateral studies that no contrast material is required. a 70 mm. camera study is also routinely performed (this involves no extra irradiation as it directly photographs the output phosphor of the image intensifier). these films are taken at 3 per sec. for 1,5 sees, using the sounds of mmh; aah; eeh and ssh to record palatal mobility and velopharyngeal closure. the films are then mounted as four strips in a plastic envelope that is conveniently kept with the patient's hospital record folder, so that comparative studies are always available. we feel that thorough long term assessment of these children will, in time, produce interesting figures of an unusual operative approach to the problem of cleft palate deformities. acknowledgement we would like to thank the medical superintendent of the red cross war memorial children's hospital for permission to publish these figures and record our gratitude to professor louw and professor cywes for their continuing encouragement. finally, we must acknowledge our debt to the murray trust fund without whose generous gifts we would not have been able to undertake this research. references 1. brown, j.b. and mcdowell, f. (1945) surgical repair of single cleft lips. surg. gynae. obstet. 80,12. 2. davies, d. (1970) the radical repair of cleft palate deformities. cte/ir palate j. 7,550. 3. longacre, j.j. (1970) cleft palate deformation: charles c. thomas. springfield, illinois. 4. millard, d.r. (1962) wide and/or short cleft palate. plast. reconstr. surg. 29,40. 5. millard, d.r. (1971) in cleft lip and palate. grabb, c.w., rosentein, s.w. and bzoch, k.r. (eds.), little, brown, boston, 6. musgrave, r.h. (1964) the unilateral cleft lip, in reconstructive plastic surgery, converse, j.m. (ed.), saunders, philadelphia. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a preliminary assessment of radical surgery in cleft palate deformities 11 7.· nordin, k.e. and johanson, β: (1955) fortschur, kiefer gesichts chir., 1,168. 8. schmid, e. (1955)fortschur, kiefer gesichts chir., 1,37. 9. schrudde, j. and stellmach, r. (1959) fortschur, kiefer gesichts chir., 5,247. 10. schweckendiek, h. (1966). primary veloplasty, in treatment of patients with clefts of lip, alveolus and palate, schuchardt, k. (ed.), georg thieme verlag, hamburg. 11. slaughter, w.b. and pruzansky, s. (1954). the rationale for velar closure as a primary procedure in the repair of cleft palate defects. plast. reconstr. surg., 13,341. 12. tennison, c.w. (1971), in cleft lip and palate. grabb, c.w., rosenstein, s.w. and bzoch, k.r., little, brown, boston. 13. trusler, h.m., bauer, t.b. and tondra, j.m. (1955). the cleft lip problem. plast. reconstr. surg. 16,174. 14. veau, v. (1938). bec-de lievre. masson et cie, paris. 15. von langenbeck's surgical procedure. (1951) in cleft lip and palate, holdsworth, w.g., heinemann medical books. 16. ward ill's surgical procedure (1951) in cleft lip and palate, holdsworth, w.g., heinemann medical books. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) s p e e c h t h e r a p y in c o l o u r e d schools, b y dorothy cornelius b.a. log. (rand) introduction. the need for a regular speech therapy service has been recognized by the transvaal education department as an essential part of education. this year a speech therapy post was specially created to cater for pupils with speech, voice and hearing defects in coloured schools. apart from facilities at the university of the witwatersrand speech, voice and hearing clinic, only meagre amenities existed for coloured speech defective children in the witwatersrand area. working conditions. the writer is at present attached to five the writer is at present attached to five schools. each school is visited approximately five times in three weeks. in addition a clinical service is run in the afternoons to cater for those children from schools which are not serviced. the clinic has its headquarters at the rand college of education for coloureds in coronationville. a classroom is used and essential items of furniture have been provided by the college. surveys of speech defectives. surveys were carried out during february and march this year. the following method was used to determine the number of speech, voice and hearing defectives at the schools: i 1. teachers were given lists with the following screening guide. (a) stuttering: excessive repetition and hesitation of words, syllables and phrases. i (b) voice disorders: e.g. voice too loud, too harsh, too soft, too high, too low, breathy. (c) articulation errors varying from complete ommission to substitution and distortion of sounds, e.g. lisp ('s' difficulty) and brei ( v difficulty). (d) severe language disturbance involving (d) comprehension and expression. (e) cleft lip and palate: speech usually very nasal. (f) hard of hearing: teachers were given 7 points to addist them to detect children in this category. approximately 60% of the teachers were able to determine the speech disorders in their classes. the remaining 40% felt they were not able to detect these defects immediately and they suggested that they observe their classes more exactly for several weeks in order to establish the cases more thoroughly. in these cases the writer did the surveys herself. during the first terms of the year the grade i classes were omitted from the surveys; except for the. obviously severe cases which had to come to the notice of teachers. 2. children were asked to count from 1 to 10, and were also asked several questions about their families, friends, hobbies etc. stutterers, cases of oral inactivity and articulatory errors were detected during these initial speech activities. results of survey. results of the survey of speech defectives carried out at 7 primary schools and 2 high schools, revealed the following data:19 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) table i. a detailed analysis of speech defectives in 9 coloured schools in johannesburg. school. no. of pupils no. with speech defects. % per school. a 389 36 9.3% β 534 40 7.5% c 546 75 14.0% d 593 70 12.0% ε 1012 117 11.0% f 311 57 18.3% g 185 35 19.0% η 992 62 6.2% i 946 60 6.3% table ii: number and percentage of different speech defects per school. no. with cleft school speech defects dysphemi s /lalia /audi a /phonia /logia palate. a36 6 (1.5%) 26 (6.7%) 3 (0.8%) 1 (0.2%) β 40 19 (3.7%) 15 (2.8%) 6 (1.1%) c 75 21 (3.8%) 42 (7.7%) 6 (1.0%) 3 (0.5%) 3 (0.5%) d 70 19 (3.2%) 37 (6.0%) 7 (1.1%) 2 (0.1%) 3 (0.5%) 2 (0.2%) ε 117 33 (3.4%) 61 (6.0%) 8 (0.8%) 6 (1.6%) 9 (0.8%) 1 (0.1%) f 57 16 (5.0%) 21 (7.0%) 12 (3.8%) 5 (0.6%) 3 (0.9%) g 35 11 (5.0%) 12 (6.4%) 6 (3.2%) 2 (1.0%) 4 (0.2%) η 62 29 (3.0%) 20(2.0%) 11 (1.1%) 2 (0.2%) i 60 16 (1.7%) 27 (2.9%) 13 (1.3%) 3 (0.3%) 1 (0.1%) table iii. total percentages of speech defects. dyslalia 47.1% dysphemia 30.8% dysaudia 13.0% dysphonia 4.0% dyslogia 4.0% cleft palate 0.9% discussion of results. from table 1 it can be seen that there is a. marked tendency for speech defectives to increase from 7.5% in the coronationville area (higher income group) to 19% in the booysens area, where the socio-economic standards are much lower. schools c and d with percentages of 14% and 12% respectively, although situated in a central area in the city, have children on their roll who live in the area known originally as western native township. formerly these families lived in the city but have since been evacuated. many of the children attending these two schools live in riverlea, fordsburg, ferreirastown and doornfontein. poor home conditions and very low standards of living in these areas may be contributing factors to the increased incidence of speech, voice and hearing defects in these schools. the two high schools included in the survey 20 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) cater for pupils from std. 6 to matric. when compared with the primary schools wide discrepancies may be noticed. the contrast in results suggests that the number of speech defects decreases over the years; either because many of the children have "out grown" their defects, or alternatively, have left the school before completing their education. the high school population is not representative of the population as a whole because children from the lower socioeconomic groups leave school earlier as compulsory education does not as yet apply to coloured schools. therefore, it would seem desirable to study speech defects in a more random sample of the adult population before making any generali zations. dysphemia. the incidence of dysphemia was found to be disturbingly high. in schools f and g the incidence is 5.1% and 5.9% respectively. in the areas where these schools are situated there is undeniable evidence of unstable or grossly irregular home life with recurrent patterms of illegitimacy, divorce and turbulent familial relationships. in school ε 3.3% of the speech defectives were stutterers and a quarter of these children were living at an orphanage. it would therefore appear that a contributory factor would be the unmet emotional needs and unsatisfactofy social relationships. in these cases stuttering might perhaps act as a vehicle to attract attention, love and affection. on the other hand it may be seen as a reaction to the strain of gross poverty and neglect. this higher incidence of stuttering among the lower socio-economic groups would seem to be the converse of the findings of other research workers in the field. from the investigations it could be concluded that stutterers were from could be concluded that stutterers were from predominantly j middle and upper class families. dyslalia. j children with articulatory disorders constitute the bulk of speech defectives. 47% of all the cases in this 'study were found to have articulatory errors, ais seen in table iii. the most common errors, are the 's', v and 'th'. errors varied from complete om'mission to substitutions of other sounds -and distortions of the particular sound. in many cases it was noted that the tendency to 'brei' (guttural v ) was a familial occurence; usually adopted from parents who invariably came from the cape districts where this is an accepted form of speech. in one family, four of the five children used to 'brei'. 'th' defects were found frequently among the afrikaans speaking children as this consonant blend is absent in afrikaans. these children often used 'de' as a substitute. dysaudia. the highest rate of hearing defects was noted in schools f and g. an explanation for this high rate may be that neglected colds lead to ear infections. because of the lack of medical and surgical treatment, some of the middle ear infections lead to permanent, hearing impairment. dyslogia. mentally defective cases constitute a great problem. because of the complete lack of special schools, a large percentage of these children are accomodated in schools for normal children. the impression was gained from discussions with a number of teachers that nothing much can be done for these backward children. they make very little progress as the classes are large and overcrowded (approximately 40 to 50 per class) and teachers are unable to give individual attention to these children. cleft palate. the incidence of cleft palate is approximately 1 in 1,000. however, no definite conclusions can be drawn as the sample was far too small. there is however reason to believe that the incidence of cleft palate is quite high in the coloured community. possible etiologial factors. in addition to the factors named above regarding possible casual factors of speech defects, the writer will venture to present a number of problems which affect the coloured community in particular, and which might possibly contribute to speech defects. in many cases speech defects were found to have no organic origin, but were a reflection of a disturbance in the whole emotional growth of the child. 21 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) children who do not talk at achool or who stutter have been found to be using various mechanisms of defence to cope with the situation at home or at school. a case in point was that of an 8 year old boy who sometimes spoke to peers in his classroom, but as soon as he was addressed by a member of the staff, he immediately became silent. an examination of conditions at home revealed that the prevailing attitude towards the children was 'children should be seen and not heard'. if the children spoke out of turn they were punished. the school itself should not be excluded as a possible factor contributing to the problem of stuttering, particularly as so many of the teachers show a complete ignorance of the problem and do not know how to treat a stutterer they may have iri their class. the child is often exposed to a teacher who is over-demanding and severe and who may con stantly ridicule and correct the child's speech. such a child may develop additional feelings of inadequacy and so aggravate the existing problem. a factor which may also promote the high incidence of speech disorders in primary schools is the abrupt plunge into formal learning without any previous preparation. the complete lack of nursery schools with trained staff does not give the child an opportunity to develop emotionally, physically and socially at his own pace.. studies have shown that where the community is not sufficiently integrated there is a greater incidence of social disorganization. since speech is viewed as an aspect of social behaviour it may be said to be a means of performing social roles. in the writers experience and general observation there appears to be very little group cohesion among members of the coloured community. there are several reasons which may be advanced for this; one of these being that members of the community have themselves created divisions in terms of class groups. for the most part these class groups are arbitrarily acquired and maintained. the following case related to the writer by a speech therapist illustrates the above point: a 7 year old boy who had previously coped adequately at school suddenly showed a marked decline in his standard of work. in addition he became very morose and withdrawn and refused to speak. to observers who were unaware of his previous performance he even seemed retarded. an examination of home conditions revealed that there were two other children in the family who were attending a european school. these children it appeared, were lighter skinned than the boy in question who was unable to pass for white. this family thus had difficulty in having him classified with the others. it was also discovered that the other two children had previously attended the coloured school from which they were later removed. this produced a tremendous emotional disturbance in the child hence his poor performance at school and his refusal to talk. some case records a few cases have been selected to illustrate etiological factors and some therapeutic aids that have proved to be effective. case 1: john, a 17 year old boy, was unable to control the pitch level of his voice. he would begin in a low pitch (suitable for his age and physical development) and then would suddenly become tense and revert to a high, tremulous voice. innitially, interviews with parents did not reveal anything significant and on ly after prolonged exploration of the history and self-concept of the case and his environment did the etiology become clear. the boy admitted that he forced himself to use the high pitch long after puberty. he was afraid that his parents would accuse him of smoking with the other teenagers in the district once his lower register became apparent. it was subsequently· found that the parents used various harsh disciplinary methods to subdue his emerging personality and were suspicious of every move and sign of independence. generally his behaviour annoyed and irritated them. in an attempt to ward off these attitudes he experienced increased anxiety and adopted behaviour patterns that were immature, embarrassing and anti-social. it was therefore necessary to give parental guidance and also build up the boy's confidence in himself. as therapy progressed he gradually gained control over his voice arid was able to relax. once he had learnt to use his new pattern consistnetly another problem arose. he was afraid to use the new voice at home because of the reaction it might evoke from his listeners. thus further home visits were made in order to gain the 22 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) parent's continued co-operation and understanding. case 2. peter, aged 7 years was the youngest of seven children. he was referred for therapy because of his stutter and additional aggressive tendencies. the interview with his guardian disclosed that he had had an extremely unstable emotional life. until recently he had lived with his parents who were about to be divorced. he had often witnessed their quarrels and violent figjits and on these occasions he would become hysterical. once his parents had separated he lived with his paternal grandmother who gave him very little affection and attention. inconsistent discipline, scolding, nagging, impatience and occasional indulgence added to his insecurity. his home conditions have considerably improved since he now lives with his maternal grandmother who is understanding and is able to build up a feeling of security. through psychodrarna and participation in a group, he has been able to free himself of much inner tension and internalized hostility. he has since made good progress. case 3: patrick, a 13 year old decondary stutterer who was completely lacking in self-confidence, had acute feelings of inadequacy and suffered increased rejection from his parents, particularly his mother, who had stuttered herself as a child and was violently aggressive towards him. at the onset of therapy he was severely emotionally disturbed and his pattern of stuttering was characterized by frequent and sudden spasms, inspiratory gasps, closing of his eyes, oscillation of the jaw, turning his head to one side and clenching his hands. his whole body was tense and rigid during each block. therapy aimed at increasing and supporting his morale by drawing attention of his assets and so changing his self-concept. his fear of stuttering was broken down by providing positive speaking situations |in which he experienced success. after a few months he had improved considerably. his teacher reported that his school work had also improved and he showed more self-assertion and initiative in class. j difficulties encountered in therapy. one of the most urgent problems facing the writer is the lack of professional consultants e.g. psychologists and neurologists, from whom to obtain additional recommendations and to whom children can be referred for differentiated tests. this is particularly important in cases of dysphasia, dyslexia etc. the achievement of accurate hearing assessment is also difficult as the only available audiometric facilities are at the johannesburg general hospital. many parents are unable to take their children in for testing as they cannot afford to lose a day's wage, by taking the time off from work. for many even the busfares are too costly and therefore the children are never tested adequately. an added difficulty is that of working in a newly established clinic where facilities are, as yet, crude. there is a lack of adequate equipment and the atmosphere of stimulation and cooperation from other therapists is lacking. need for educating the community regarding the role of speech therapy. greater recognition needs to be given by the community to the value of speech therapy. here the school can play an important role as it has a recognized status. it is seen as an educational institution and as such its activities are accepted as furthering the aims of education which the community desires. many parents are as yet suspicious of the therapist's intentions as they have not previously encountered this aspect of education. speech therapy is often regarded as 'learning to talk snobbishly' and the significance of the service as a remedial innovation is not clearly understood. by its inclusion as a free school service and the facilities of parental counselling which it offers, many of these barriers may be broken down. in addition, positive promotion by the school administration and talks to parent-teacher associations will increase the co-operation and recognition which is so urgently needed, if the speech therapist is to fulfill her function to the community. conclusions. the school setting provides opportunities for early detection of speech problems and for work 23 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) of a remedial nature. it is often an effective medium for reaching the child who has both speech and emotional problems. in numerous cases speech problems appeared to be tied up with unfavourable home conditions. thus it is essential to work with all those factors which have a bearing on the child's development, i.e. the family, school and social milieu. although no definite conclusions can be drawn at this stage, the study does seem to point out that a larger percentage of speech defects were found among children from the lower socioeconomic groups. this work has been of an exploratory nature and it is felt that many challenging aspects regarding the coloured community are open to further research. in this paper only pertinent problems have been briefly outlined. summary. for the first time this year a post for a speech therapist was made available to cater for the needs of speech defectives in coloured schools. results of the survey suggest that a large percentage of children require speech therapy. a detailed analysis of the results has been given in tabular form. the writer has ventured to give a number of possible etiological factors and has also presented some of the problems which appear to affect the coloured community in particular. opsommihg. vir die eerste keer, is daar gedurende hierdie jaar 'n pos vir 'n spraakterapeut geskep, sodat aan. die behoeftes van die nie-blanke skoolkind voldoen kan word. die resultate van die opname dui op 'n hoe persentasie van kinders wat spraakterapie benodig. 'n uitvoerige ontleding van die resultate is in tabelvorm gegee. die skrywer het gepoog om verskeie etiologiese faktore vas te stel en het verder ook gedui op sekere probleme wat blykbaar net betrekking het op die nie-blanke gemeenskap. 24 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) profiles o f outpatient speech-language therapy and a udiology clients at a tertiary hospital in the western cape 49 profiles of outpatient speech-language therapy and audiology clients at a tertiary hospital in the western cape w endy overett and harsha kathard division o f communication sciences and disorders, university o f cape town a b s t r a c t this stu d y d e s c r ib e d trends in the dem ographic, diagn ostic, a n d attendan ce va ria b le s o f first-tim e clien ts who a tten d ed the speech l angu age therapy a n d a u d io lo g y o u tp a tien t d ep a rtm en t (o p d ) a t a te rtia ry h o sp ita l in the w estern c ape o v e r a f iv e -y e a r p e r io d (19992003). a retro sp ective, d e scrip tive su rve y w a s co n d u cted a n d d ata w ere c o lle c te d fro m h o sp ita l records. the d a ta w e re coded, a n a lyse d d es crip tive ly a n d re p rese n ted in tables, fig u r e s a n d graph s. the d ia g n o stic c a teg o ries o f h earin g loss a n d v o ic e d iso rd e r m ade up the h ighest p e r c e n ta g e o f referrals. the la rg e st p e r c e n ta g e s o f clien ts w ere re fe rred fr o m the ear, n o se a n d throat (ent) d ep a rtm en t a t the stu d y hospital, f o llo w e d b y the edu cation sector. d em o g ra p h ic trends in d ica ted th a t the h ig h est p e r c e n ta g e o f clien ts a tten din g the o p d w as in the a g e ca teg o ry o f 7-12 ye a rs, that sim ila r p e r c e n ta g e s o f m a le a n d fe m a le clien ts a tte n d e d the opd, an d th at english w a s the language th a t a clien t w as m o st likely to speak. g eograph ically, the highest p e r c e n ta g e o f clien ts a tten d in g the o p d w a s fr o m the k lip fo n tein region. a tten d a n ce d a ta r e v e a le d th a t a p p ro x im a tely 70% o f clien ts a tte n d e d o p d appoin tm ents within a three-m onth tim e p e ­ rio d a n d a tten d ed no m ore than tw o appointm ents. p erc en ta g e o f appoin tm ents a tte n d e d d e c r e a s e d with in creasin g num bers o f sch ed ­ uled appointm ents. im p lica tio n s f o r research a n d se rv ic e d e liv e ry a re discussed. key words: speech-language therapy and audiology services, healthcare, service delivery, client profiles, attendance introduction this study aimed to describe the client population profiles of those attending the speech-language therapy and audiology outpatient department (opd) at a tertiary hospital in the western cape in south africa over a five year period (1999-2003). to achieve this aim, the demographic, diagnostic and attendance data of a study sample were determined. the impetus for this study arose from difficulties the researcher experienced while attempt­ ing to engage with service planning for speech-language ther­ apy services in a post-apartheid era at a tertiary hospital in the absence o f knowledge o f descriptive client data. in the south african public sector, slt & audiology ser­ vices have traditionally been located in both the health and educa­ tion sectors. in the health sector these services have been consid­ ered as part o f allied health service delivery with emphasis on rehabilitation (allied health professionals technical committee, 2004). although health services are essential, they fail to reach many people in south africa due to a lack o f resources. histori­ cally, health service delivery in! south africa has been shaped by the medical model and complied with apartheid ideology (bhagwanjee & stewart, 1999; hall, haynes & mccoy, 2002). public health services were characterised by racial and geographi­ cal disparities, fragmentation and duplication o f services and were hospital-centred with minimal emphasis on primary health care (phc). the combined influences o f the medical model and apart­ heid ideology resulted in an inequitable provision o f services in terms o f accessibility, appropriateness, funding and co-ordination across the variables o f race, class, gender and level o f urbanisa­ tion (bhagwanjee & stewart, 1999). the healthcare system was divided, inefficient and grossly inequitable (hall et al., 2002). speech-language therapy (slt) and audiology services in south africa have been shaped by such socio-political contex­ tual realities and are also grossly inequitable (pillay, kathard & samuel, 1997). services in south africa have been biased toward providing a better quality service to a white, middle class, eng­ lish and afrikaans first language speaking population, whilst pro­ viding a poorer service to a black african first language speaking clientele (pillay, 1996 cited in pillay et al., 1997). public sector slt & audiology services have historically been based in hospi­ tals and special schools, with minimal service delivery at commu­ nity level. similar to the national profile, services in the western cape have been urban-based with few speech-language thera­ pists and audiologists working in rural areas beyond the cape metropole. health sector provision o f slt & audiology services have been limited to services at tertiary hospitals, with little avail­ able at primary or secondary levels in the western cape (allied health professionals technical committee, 2004), thus limiting access and availability o f services to the majority o f the popula­ tion. the current service delivery situation is untenable given the prevalence o f disability in the western cape. the data from the 2001 census (statistics south africa, 2004) regarding the numbers o f people with communication, hearing or multiple dis­ abilities point to the need for slt & audiology services in the city o f cape town and in the western cape. the need for acces­ sible services, coupled with appropriate planning, and importantly the knowledge to assist such planning, is essential to meet the service delivery needs o f this population. over the last decade, systemic changes have taken place within the health system which have had a bearing on slt & audiology services. subsequent to south africa's first democratic elections in 1994, the national department o f health set about transforming and restructuring the healthcare system (hall et al., 2002; forman, pillay & sait, 2004). these changes included a policy shift to a primary health care (phc) approach within a dis­ trict health system (dhs), with intent to realise a social model of health. the aim o f such transformation has been to improve the quality o f care provided and create a more equitable service. there is a critical need for relevant knowledge to inform such planning processes. in order to ensure cost-effective improvement and development o f existing health services, the efficacy o f health programmes needs to be validated through research showing that such services are necessary, appropriate and accessible, within the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 50 wendy overett and harsha kathard specified contexts (bhagwanjee & stewart, 1999). several studies within the slt & audiology profession, have examined client profiles to facilitate service planning. inter­ nationally studies by broomfield and dodd (2004); edwards, cape and brown (1989); enderby and davies (1989); enderby and petheram (2000); heron (2001); and petheram and enderby (2001) have provided critical data to influence service planning. in south africa a limited number o f studies have examined client data in different contexts (klop, 1998; schneider, 1992 and swanepoel, 2005). klop (1998) conducted a study in the area o f quality man­ agement in a private healthcare practice in cape town. the out­ comes o f the study were intended to help implement quality man­ agement programmes. client profiles in terms o f disorders, age, geographical location, gender, home language and referral agents were compiled from 197 clients seen by the researcher in her prac­ tice from 1994 to 1996. she found that the majority o f clients at­ tended for language disorders, followed by stuttering, almost half were between three and six years o f age, 63% o f the clients were nale, almost three quarters o f her clients were afrikaans-speaking and most o f the clients were drawn from an area within a 10 km radius o f the practice. her clients were referred from other health­ care professionals, educators and former clients. from a smaller group o f clients she found that only two out o f 64 clients withdrew from therapy. schneider (1992) collected data from records, reports and case files to determine the nature and prevalence o f communica­ tion disorders seen in six hospitals in gazankulu. community based speech and hearing workers were then interviewed about their work situation, organisation of their time and intervention strategies used with communicatively disordered people in order to evaluate the efficacy o f their work. the most common disorders seen at the hospitals in gazankulu by these speech and hearing workers were hearing disorders. swanepoel (2005) conducted an exploratory descriptive study which critically described an infant hearing screening pro­ gramme conducted at two maternal and child health clinics in hammanskraal. quantitative and qualitative methods for data col ! lection were used his study showed poor follow-up return rates i to the clinic. | in south africa, apart from these studies by klop (1998), f schneider (1992) and swanepoel (2005) there is limited data to contribute to service planning. the unique characteristics and challenges o f developing countries demand that contextual and local research be done in conjunction with international .studies (swanepoel, 2005). speech-language therapy research in the united kingdom has emphasised the importance of ongoing gathering information on services and client populations for speech-language therapy services. as with south africa, inequalities in services for the speech and language impaired in the united kingdom’s national health service have been documented (enderby & davies, 1989). if there is to be efficient planning in healthcare it is important that this process be informed with regard to the size and needs o f the population to be served (enderby & davies, 1989). the collation of client data profiles may serve in questioning some o f the basic premises regarding provision o f services and should lead to con­ sideration o f alternative forms of provision which meet the needs o f the speech and/or language disabled and hearing impaired popu­ lation (enderby, 1989). to monitor equity concerns in south africa, comparative data are necessary to understand the multifaceted nature o f client profiles which include race, age, gender, urban/rural location and socio-economic status (ntuli & day, 2004). the first steps how­ ever, are to collect data from current service sites, as was the inten­ tion o f the present study. the demographic, diagnostic and atten­ dance data o f clients receiving services at a tertiary level o f health­ care could be valuable in planning equitable and appropriate ser­ vices. reliable data concerning the types o f speech, language and hearing disorders referred, age and sources o f referral, and the ef­ fect o f cultural and socio-economic profiles o f the population on referral patterns are vital for planning services (broomfield & dodd, 2004). comparison o f records may show service trends, admission patterns and familial trends (lubker & tomblin, 1998) over time. it was important to collect data gathered over time so that changes in service provision could be examined with the pur­ pose o f informing debate and planning (petheram & enderby, 2001). the compilation and analysis o f data, timeous reporting and use o f consistent up-to-date health information, are all key aspects o f healthcare planning and management. the lack o f information regarding the health sector has contributed significantly to the slow process o f transforming the health system (ijumba & day, 2004). thus the results o f this study, with its focus on demographic, diag­ nostic and attendance data, has potential to influence service deliv­ ery changes with regard to slt & audiology services. methodology aim this study aimed to describe trends in the demographic, diagnostic, and attendance variables of first-time clients who at­ tended the slt & audiology outpatient department (opd) at the tertiary hospital being studied over a five year time period (1999 2003). study design a retrospective descriptive survey was conducted, to collect demographic, diagnostic and attendance data o f a sample o f clients. sample i the study population included all the hospital records o f new clients who had attended the speech-language therapy and audiology opd o f the tertiary hospital under study during a 5 year period from january 1, 1999 to december 31, 2003. this period of time was selected as data were readily available and would give a sufficiently large number o f records for trends over the years to be examined. due to the large number o f clients in the population (2,819), a sample o f one-third o f the population data were drawn, using a stratified random sampling procedure (katzenellenbogen, joubert & abdool karim, 1997), stratified according to the year in which the client was first seen. the sample consisted of 929 cli­ ents, on which the analysis was conducted. data collection and analysis the clients' names were retrieved from the client registers and record cards. the data collection form, devised by the re­ searcher, was then utilized to capture the data from the hospital records (appendix a). data were electronically captured onto a spreadsheet as raw data and then entries were coded by the re­ searcher. in instances where data were missing on the form (where it was not indicated as missing in the records), the records were die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) profiles o f outpatient speech-language therapy and a udiology clients at a tertiary hospital in the western cape 51 reviewed again by the researcher to retrieve this information. after the data had been entered into the computer and checked, client names and folder numbers were removed to ensure anonymity o f the clients. attendance data were then converted to number o f appointments attended, time frame over which these appointments were attended and the percentage o f appointments attended. the co d ed data were transferred from the spreadsheet into a computer driven statistical package (statistical package for the social sci­ ences) (spss inc, 1995) and frequency and percentage distribu­ tions as well as contingency tables were generated. in instances where a data collection form had missing data, after being re-checked by the researcher, information which was unavailable, was categorised as such. if a record for a client had missing data for two or three o f the three diagnostic variables, the record was excluded from analysis o f diagnostic variables and at­ tendance variables, but was still included in the analysis o f demo­ graphic variables. in instances where there was no clear atten­ dance data for two or more attendance variables, that client was excluded from the analysis o f attendance information. however, if only one o f these variables was missing, the data were still in­ cluded in the overall attendance analysis. due to the descriptive nature o f this study, analysis of variables was conducted via descriptive statistics (hite, 2001), as used in other similar studies (enderby & petheram, 2000; petheram & enderby, 2001). frequency and percentage distributions were used to describe the demographic, diagnostic and attendance data. frequency data were converted into percentage data for each year and for the total sample. contingency tables (cross tabulation) were used to show relationships between variables. inferential statistics were not used as this form o f data analysis is not appropriate for a descriptive study (hite, 2001). reliability and validity reliability o f the data collection procedure was ensured by defining the exact methods o f measuring and checking of work (katzenellenbogen, et al., 1997). in this study a standard, reliable data collection form (appendix a) was used to collect data. cross­ checks o f information from the hospital record sheet to the slt & audiology folder also enhanced j the reliability o f the data collec­ tion process. | areas o f concern for validity included the reality that hospital records are generally produced for clinical, administrative or monetary ends rather than research purposes (abramson, 1990) and that different clinicians ma>| have used different terminology when describing diagnoses. terminologies were therefore exam­ ined and then grouped in broad categories. data collection was verified by the researcher as it was entered into the computer. instrument, observer and client variations can be evaluated by repeating measures o f a sub-sample o f the study sample (katzenellenbogen, et al., 1997). in this study a random sample of ten percent o f the clients from each year was selected for re­ collection o f data (n=93) by an independent researcher. the data were then coded and the codes were compared to those found for the same clients by the researcher. there was 95% agreement across all the variables, indicating good inter-observer reliability. a large sample was chosen to enhance the validity o f the study as confidence intervals become narrower as sample size in­ creases (katzenellenbogen, et al., 1997). the data were analysed quantitatively using spss to ensure the data were analysed in a consistent manner thereby ensuring valid and reliable results. the results were interpreted by the researcher who also engaged with a process o f peer review to enhance the quality o f the interpretation. ethical considerations the head o f the speech-language therapy and audiology department, the medical superintendent of the tertiary hospital be­ ing studied, and the ethics committee o f the university o f cape town were consulted with regard to gaining access to the hospital files. it was not, however, possible to gain consent from the cli­ ents, due to potential difficulties locating clients and the number of records being reviewed. however, confidentiality was strictly maintained by removing any identifying information after the data had been checked and coded. access to the files may be consid­ ered ethical on the grounds that there was no risk o f harm to indi­ viduals, there was potential public benefit, and investigators' pro­ tected the confidentiality o f the individuals whose data they stud­ ied (council for international organisations o f medical sciences, 1991). the community will stand to benefit from this research if changes, taking into account the findings o f this study, are made within the healthcare setting with regard to equity in service provi­ sion and aligning the services to the needs o f the population. results and discussion the number o f clients varied each year ranging from 178 to 197 clients, with a total sample size o f 929 clients on which these results are based. the results and discussion are presented in rela­ tion to the aims o f the study. the total number of clients varied across the aims because o f missing information as explained in the methodology. the quantitative results are represented in tables and graphs and the main trends are described and then discussed. diagnostic data speech-language therapy and audiology diagnosis ; in this study recorded reported medical diagnoses have been managed as inter-related diagnostic categories with slt & audiology diagnoses. the percentage of clients within each slt & audiology diagnostic category is presented in figure 1. j speech-language therapy and audiology diagnostic categories o f j those attending the opd appeared to remain fairly consistent over the five-year period, with minor variations across the years. ! j the common diagnoses o f those attending the slt & audi­ ology opd included hearing loss (n=255; 30%), voice disorders (n=l 77; 21%) and neurogenic communication disorders. the ; types and percentages o f diagnoses remained relatively similar across the five years. there are several significant issues surrounding the types o f disorders seen at the slt & audiology opd and the percentage of clients attending the opd for the management o f each disorder (figure 1). the reason for the high percentages o f clients attend­ ing the opd as a result o f a hearing loss or voice disorders, may be due to the fact that this slt & audiology opd is situated in a tertiary hospital, with specialized equipment and facilities suitable for assessing and treating these disorders. due to the fact that hearing loss and voice disorders, as well as neurogenic communication disorders (for which the third highj est percentage of clients attended the opd) are medically based and are treated by the medical profession in conjunction with speech-language therapists and audiologists, clients may be likely to attend this hospital which offers both services. hearing loss, voice and neurogenic communication disorders are often ac­ quired and therefore there is a ‘loss’ o f function, making the disor­ der noticeable and igniting a desire or need to restore lost function. the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 52 wendy overett and harsha kathard v s p e e c h a n d a u d i o l o g y d i a g n o s is figure 1: percentage of clients per speech-language therapy and audiology diagnostic category who attended the speech-language therapy and audiology opd at a tertiary hospital in the western cape from 1999-2003 (n=859). the proximity and connections to the ent department, who made the largest percent­ age of referrals to the slt & audiology opd, may also account for the high per­ centages of clients who attended the opd as a result of a hearing loss or voice disor­ der. the highest percentages of clients were diagnosed with hearing losses, voice disorders, neurogenic communication dis­ orders or fluency disorders, which are ‘noticeable’ disorders and thus easier to detect than less ‘visible’ disorders (mclaren, solarsh & saloojee, 2004) and are likely to impact on everyday life. less ‘visible’ disorders, such as a language im­ pairment, may not be as easily noticeable in everyday situations and thus not as eas­ ily detected. these less ‘visible’ disorders are then not as likely to be referred to slt & audiology services. it is also possible that because the department is actually referred to as a “speech therapy” rather than “speech-language therapy”, it may not be obviously associated with managing language disorders, a reality that the pro­ fession has experienced internationally. referral sources the results for referral sources ap­ pear in figure 2. from the year 2000 on­ wards there was an increase in referrals from community clinics and from wards at the study hospital. when examining individual cate­ gories of clients with a specific disorder or from a specific referral source, particu­ lar trends were found. these results are based on the clients within a specific cate­ gory only and thus the percentages and numbers are based on these individual categories and not on the full sample. the majority of clients who attended the slt & audiology opd as a result of a voice disorders (n=137; 76%) were re­ ferred from the ent department. fifty percent (n=41) of referrals from commu­ nity clinics, 80.5% (n=33) o f referrals from occupational health centres and 83% (n= 10) of referrals from old age homes or residential care were referred to the slt & audiology opd as a result of a hearing loss. the majority o f the referrals to the opd from wards were for dysphagia, neurogenic communication disorders or a combination of these (n=31; 97%). the education sector referred 50% (n=28) of paediatric clients with multiple difficulties to the opd. referral sources (figure 2) would also have shaped the client diagnostic pro­ files discussed above. as mentioned, the ent department made a high percentage of referrals to the slt & audiology opd. this trend is understandable in light of the discussion around the types of disorders seen most frequently at the opd. the second major referral agent to the opd was the education sector. this situation may be attributed to the limited slt & audiology services within mainstream schools (f. lewis, personal communica­ tion, february 18, 2005), resulting in re­ ferrals to the health sector. j i demographic data | i age groups age group results, presented in table 1, illustrate that the largest percent­ age (n=l 65; 18%) of clients who attended the opd were within the age group of 7 12 years. the following results are based on sub-categories of clients. clients attend­ ing the opd with a hearing loss or tinni­ tus covered the full range of ages, al­ though the majority of these clients were between 30 and 79 years o f age (n= 181; 72%). the majority of clients who at­ tended the slt & audiology opd as a result of a voice disorder were between 30 t t& cs. " \\\% x v x r e f e r r a l s o u r c e <> '■v figure 2: percentage of clients per referral source who attended the speech-language therapy and audiology opd at a tertiary hospital in the western cape from 1999 2003 (n=859). a bbreviations used: c t : c a p e t ow n oa h : o ld a ge h om es en t: e ar, n ose, t hroat die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) profiles o f outpatient speech-language therapy and a udiology clients at a tertiary hospital in the western cape 53 ancj 49 years o f age (n=98; 54%). clients with a neurogenic com­ munication disorder spanned a wide range o f ages, although the highest percentage fell within the age group o f 50-59 years (n=27; 24%). the majority o f clients who attended the opd as a result of dysphagia were between 50 and 69 years of age (n=10; 67%), while clients with both dysphagia and a neurogenic communica­ tion disorder were slightly older, between 60 and 79 years (n=23; 62%). the majority of clients presenting with fluency disorders, however, were under 18 years o f age (n=64; 90%). a similar trend occurred for clients attending as a result o f a phonology/ articulation/oral motor disorder, language disorder, language and learning disorder or multiple difficulties although the majority were under the age o f 12 (n=20; 87%). while the highest percent­ age o f clients attending as a result o f language disorders was in the under six year age group (n=15; 65%), children attending the slt & audiology opd with language and learning disorders or multi­ ple difficulties had a greater percentage in the 7-12 year old cate­ gory (n=25; 83% and n=35; 62.5% respectively). despite the high number o f clients with a hearing loss or voice disorder attending the opd (which in this study were found predominantly in the adult population), the majority o f the caseload attending the slt & audiology opd was found to be between 7 and 12 years o f age (table 1). o f this age group only a minority attended the opd for a hearing loss or voice disorder. however, this age group had the majority o f the clients with flu­ ency disorders, phonology/articulation/oral motor disorders, lan­ guage and learning disorders, as well as those with multiple diffi­ culties. a reason for this high percentage o f 7 to 12 year olds at­ tending the opd under study in relation to the under six popula­ tion, may be due to the close proximity o f another tertiary hospital, which specializes in paediatric intervention. the high percentage of 7 to 12 year olds attending the opd (who are primary school age children) alludes to the lack o f services within the education system, who were also the second highest referral agent. despite the inclusive education policy intention regarding inclusive educa­ tion (department of education, 2001), there seems to have been minimal support for learners with communication difficulties in mainstream schools in the western cape. the researcher has also perceived a general lack o f knowltable 1: number and percentage of clients per age group who attended the speech-language therapy and audiology opd at a tertiary hospital in the western cape from 1999-2003 (n=910) j age number percent 0 6 years 69 : 7.58 7-12 years 165 18.13 13-18 years 39 4.29 19-29 years 79 8 .6 8 30-39 years 118 12.97 40-49 years 115 12.64, 50-59 years 107 11.76 60-69 years 101 1 1 .1 0 70-79 years 87 9.56 80+ years 30 3.30 total 910 ' 100 edge amongst educators, many health professionals and the public, o f slt & audiology services, resulting in ‘late’ referrals o f indi­ viduals. anecdotal parent reports have indicated that parents who had voiced their concerns regarding their child’s delayed language development have been told by health professionals ‘not to worry’ or that their child will ‘grow out o f it’. thus, instead o f the child receiving early intervention, intervention was only available upon entering school where there are high demands on communication skills and the child’s difficulties begin to impact on academic per­ formance. this scenario again relates to the ‘visibility’ o f certain disorders (mclaren et al., 2004), as these language and learning disorders are often only detected in an academic environment and are not immediately obvious in everyday situations (das, 2001). examination o f the results pertaining to the adult popula­ tion attending the speech-language therapy and audiology opd showed that there was a slight decrease in percentage o f clients attending the opd within each 10 year category from 30 to 69 years, with a slightly larger decrease in therapy attendance at 70 to79 years and a large decrease in therapy attendance in the over 80 year old category. an explanation for the greatest number o f adults attending the opd being between 30 and 49 years o f age may be due to the large number o f clients attended the opd as a result o f a voice disorder and the fact that the majority o f clients with voice disorders were in this age group. this finding o f a decrease in therapy attendance amongst clients with increasing age, however, seems to follow a general trend in rehabilitation, where services are less accessible and avail­ able to the older population. although disability is more prevalent in the older population, it seems that older adults are unable to access hospital based outpatient services. blake (1981) in the united states o f america, claims that in terms o f age group, par­ ticipation in the rehabilitation service seems to be inversely related to the need for such service. rehabilitation may be more o f a pri­ ority for younger people, or services may be more accessible to them, than for the older population. rehabilitation has previously been driven both philosophically and financially by the goal o f restoring individuals to productive employment, resulting in better opportunities for younger clients, although this is now changing to include a focus on older clients (raia, 1992). the motivation to attend rehabilitation may be greater for the younger adults due to their need to regain employment, as well as to pursue social goals. a study at groote schuur hospital in the western cape, south africa (whitelaw, meyer, bawa & jennings, 1994) confirmed this trend and reported that greater numbers o f stroke clients under the age o f 65 years old, who had been inpatients, were referred (as outpatients) to physiotherapy and occupational therapy services, while fewer clients above the age o f 65 were referred. further­ more, a greater number o f the younger adults presented for and received therapy than older adults. another study in the western cape, examining rehabilitation services at bishop lavis rehabili­ tation centre, documented that 50% o f the population attending these services were 60 years old or younger (rhoda, 2001). in light o f the above discussion regarding services for dif­ ferent age groups o f clients, the current study revealed that a large percentage (n=71; 64%) o f clients with a neurogenic communica­ tion disorder (without dysphagia) were under the age o f 60 years. however, 70% (n=26) o f those with both dysphagia and a neuro­ genic communication disorder were over the age of 60. this pos­ sibly indicates a greater degree o f disability in the older age groups, where multiple disabilities are perhaps more likely. this trend o f multiple disabilities in the older population may also ex­ plain the reduction in the number of these older clients attending outpatient rehabilitation. elderly clients may not only be more the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 54 wendy overett and harsha kathard affected by a stroke, but may also have more problems reaching therapists than younger clients (whitelaw et al., 1994). there is also greater attrition of older clients than younger clients due to stroke (whitelaw et al., 1994). gender slightly more than half of the sample who attended the opd was male (n=470; 52%). more females than males presented with voice disorders, language disorders or no specific speech-language &/or audiology diagnosis. it was unexpected to find similar percentages of male and female clients attending the opd, as the belief was that there would be many more males than females as found in other studies (for ex­ ample broomfield & dodd, 2004; enderby & petheram, 2000; klop, 1998), although direct comparisons could not be drawn as some studies did not include audiology clients and some were based only on paediatric populations. this trend, however, be­ comes clearer when one considers gender in relation to the different disorders. as found in the present study, as well as other studies (coyle, weinrich & stemple, 2001), voice disorders are more prevalent amongst females than males. this study found that a large percentage of clients (n=180; 21%) attended the opd for the management of a voice disorder and because many o f these clients were female the number of females attending the slt & audiology opd was increased. there are two possible explanations as to why there were more female than male language disorders found in this study. firstly, more males presented with language disorders coupled with learning disorders and were thus categorised as such. secondly, it was found that more of the language disorders fell into the 0-6 year old category. there is a perception that ‘boys develop more slowly’ or ‘boys are slower to talk’ (phillips, 2004:500) and they might therefore be brought later for therapy, when they would more likely be diagnosed as having a language and learning difficulty. of all the other disorders (apart from language disorders) found mostly in children, all had higher percentages of males than females. many studies have commented on the greater likelihood of reading difficulties or dyslexia in males than in females (owens, 1999) and that children with dyslexia have delayed language devel­ opment. also, as expected, more males than females attended the opd for the intervention of fluency disorders (guitar, 1998). home language the majority of clients attending the opd were classified as having english or afrikaans as their home language, with a minor­ ity speaking xhosa or another language (table 2). table 2: number and percentage of clients per language group who attended the speech-language therapy and audiology opd at a tertiary hospital in the western cape from 1999-2003 (n=910) this is interesting considering that 55% of the population in the western cape speak afrikaans as a home language, 24% speak xhosa as a home language and 19% speak english as a home lan­ guage (south africa.info reporter, 2002). there are several possi­ ble reasons for the mismatch between the population profile and the profile of clients attending the slt & audiology opd. firstly, english may be the preferred language of interven­ tion for several reasons. the majority of speech-language thera­ pists and audiologists in the slt & audiology opd at this tertiary hospital in the western cape were english first language speaking with afrikaans as a second language. also, many children were being schooled in english, as english was the language most com­ monly used as the medium of instruction (alant, 1989). thus they may choose to receive therapy in english. thirdly, there were no xhosa-speaking speech-language therapists or audiologists at the study hospital and no formal access to interpreters, resulting in lim­ ited provision of services to the xhosa-peaking population. this situation is of grave concern given the multilingual nature of the population in the region. another possible reason for xhosa-speaking clients not ac­ cessing services in the opd clinic may be due to cultural reasons. cultural groups vary in their view of disability and therapeutic in­ terventions and such differences may impact who is likely to attend the hospital (swartz, 1998; swanepoel, 2005). furthermore, the hospital in the present study has historically been a ‘white’ hospital, and there may still be misconceptions about who can access ser­ vices at this hospital. in many communities in south africa there is little awareness of slt & audiology services, due to lack of re­ sources and facilities, and therefore people are unaware that ser­ vices are available. access to services is also influenced by socioeconomic status of people. although poverty is not confined to one racial group in south africa, it is most prevalent amongst the black popu­ lation (swanepoel, 2005). many of the xhosa-speaking population in the western cape, the majority of whom would be black, may have a limited income. the costs of travel to the hospital as well as multiple treatment fees may be prohibitive. geographical location the health districts in cape town were used to categorise the areas from where the clients who attended the opdlcame. i table 3: number and percentage of clients per geographical location who attended the speech-language therapy and audiology opd at a tertiary hospital in the western cape from 1999-2003 (n=910) geographical location number percent central 209 22.97 eastern 2 2 2.42 khayelitsha 39 4.29 klipfontein 258 28.35 mitchells plain 117 1 2 .8 6 northern panorama 46 5.05 southern 150 16.48 tygerberg 39 4.29 outside cape town . 21 2.31 m issing data 9 0.99 total 910 10 0 language for therapy number percent english 501 55.05 afrikaans 298 32.75 xhosa 82 9.01 other 18 1.98 m issing data 11 1.21 total 910 1 0 0 .0 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) profiles o f outpatient speech-language therapy and audiology clients at a tertiary hospital in the western cape 55 table 3 shows the number and percentage of clients from each area. it can be seen that clients attended from many different areas. this is not sur­ prising given the lack of slt & audiol­ ogy services in the community or even at a secondary level of care (allied health professionals technical commit­ tee, 2004). some of the areas, however, would have been covered by slt & audiology services at another tertiary hospital in cape town. attendance the majority of clients attended the opd over a very short period of time, for example many attended within a three month period (n=513; 71%) or attended two appointments (n=506; 70%). number of appointments and time frame results showed very similar trends, with a drop-off with increasing number of scheduled appointments or longer period of time over which these appointments were scheduled. almost half of all the clients (n=353; 49%) at­ tended only one appointment. only 9% (n=67) of clients attended more than five appointments. given that long-term therapy was often indicated, these find­ ings were noteworthy in relation to ser­ vice planning. the high number of clients who only had one booked appointment may be due to the fact that certain clients would have been placed on a waiting list for therapy and therefore not been given further scheduled appointments immedi­ ately. they may then not have wanted therapy when it was offered, due to changes in circumstances between the time of the assessment and the date when therapy was offered. additionally, there are some cases for which one attendance may be all that is indicated. some clients may have needed an initial assessment with no necessary follow-up. it is likely that once their concerns had been ad­ dressed, they no longer felt the need for further intervention. enderby and da­ vies (1989) reported that 60% of newly referred children required assessment and advice only. while this explanation might be applicable to some cases, the data in case files, the nature of the disor­ ders and personal experience suggest that many required additional intervention. the lack of understanding of the nature of interventions offered by the profession of slt & audiology by the community, as well as the location of the services within a tertiary hospital envi­ ronment, may have contributed to poor attendance rates. many clients have the expectation that a hospital is a place which one attends when ill and that it is a once-off attendance, possibly with a fol­ low-up appointment. many expect treat­ ment that is tangible, such as medication or surgery. clients arrive with the ex­ pectation that they will be given some­ thing to ‘cure’ them in a once-off ap­ pointment. therefore, they may not have been prepared for the active role that is required of them in the therapeutic proc­ ess. also, they may have been unwilling or unable to commit themselves to fur­ ther intervention as recommended. the high rate of non-attendance may also reflect the economically disadvantaged nature of the population served, and the low priority given to slt & audiology services given other life circumstances (broomfield & dodd, 2004). a further explanation for low at­ tendance rates could relate to different perceptions of a disorder by a therapist and a client, or the client having a differ­ ent understanding of the nature of a dis­ order. negotiating between explana­ tions of illness increases the possibilities of compliance or adherence to treatment (swartz, 1998). thus, explaining the nature of the diagnosis in ways that the client can understand and relate to their understanding of the world, may im­ prove attendance and compliance to therapy. also, discussions prior to com­ mencing therapy about the nature of therapy and the respective roles of the parent and the therapist may well in­ crease parent satisfaction (roulstone, glogowska, peters & enderby, 2004), which may improve attendance rates for children. the same principle could be applied to adults. with an increasing number of scheduled appointments, percentage of appointments attended dropped. the only diagnostic categories which had more than 10% of clients attending in excess of five appointments were for the categories o f fluency disorders (n=15; 28%), paediatrics with multiple difficul­ ties (n=12; 25%), language disorders (n=5; 24%) and phonology/ articulation/ oral motor disorders (n=3; 13%). it is possible that the motivation for treat­ ment of these disorders may have been greater than for other disorders. data presented by enderby and davies (1989), which included many but not all types of communication disorders included in this study, indicated that the children who received regular therapy, attended once a week for an average of 16 weeks. roulstone et al. (2004) found that of the 71 children allocated to speech-language therapy in their study, 68 attended sessions offered, al­ though nearly 18% of appointments were either cancelled (by the therapist or clinician) or not attended. klop (1998) in her private practice found that only two out of 64 clients dropped out of therapy. the differences in findings be­ tween these studies confirm the impor­ tance of understanding how contextual realities influence service provision. 4 0 t im e p eriod figure 3: percentage of clients who attended their scheduled appointments at the speech-language therapy and audiology opd at a tertiary hospital in the western cape from 1999-2003 across different time periods (n=719). the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 56 wendy overett and harsha kathard limitations there are several limitations of this study which need to be considered. there has been minimal research into client profiles attending slt & audiology services in south africa. therefore, the researcher had to rely on the methodology and literature from other countries and other healthcare disciplines to conduct this study. while such literature was useful, it was not directly appli­ cable to the slt & audiology contexts in south africa. the study was also limited to the data from one tertiary hospital examined over a five year time period. the conclusions therefore are only applicable to the tertiary hospital being studied during the speci­ fied time period. due to the nature of the study, records used were not de­ vised specifically for research purposes and thus may not have been maintained with the care that would be expected in a planned investigation. notably, as diagnostic labels vary in use across slt & audiology services and medical categories, specific diagnoses are open to different interpretations. for example, a patient’s pri­ mary medical diagnoses may be a “cerebral-vascular accident” with slt & audiology services referring to “neurogenic acquired communication disorders” . this resulted in difficulties during the data collection phase which included missing records, incomplete records, and information recorded in an inconsistent manner within and between records. this is a common limitation of retrospective studies such as those by farmer (1990) and schneider (1992). a further problem associated with the description of communication disorders is that many disorders (for example learning difficulties) are not clearly defined (lubker, 1997). limitations in the interpretation of the results included lack of comparison data from other sites, limited ability to generalize the findings of this research to other service sites, and that the in­ terpretation was speculative in nature. due to the descriptive na­ ture of the study, no causal or correlational assertions could be drawn (hite, 2001). despite such limitations, the findings can in­ form the restructuring of services in the region. the difficulties encountered with the use of records points to the need for good record keeping, which is essential for plan­ ning of healthcare services (rhoda, 2001). inadequate records can distort research results, prevent clients from being involved in re­ search and negate the quality of research (farmer, 1990). there­ fore, methods of recording client information and treatments used need to be reviewed (farmer, 1990). this information is not only useful for research, but also when assessing cost-efficiency of a service (rhoda, 2001). the limited access to slt & audiology services in the western cape, as reflected in the poor attendance rates obtained in this study, point to the need for slt & audiology services at all levels, and particularly at a primary level, of care (rhoda, 2001). services need to be provided in line with the primary health care approach within the district health system (hall et al., 2002; for­ man, et al., 2004). ideally, the services should be integrated at all levels of care and should strive towards community based rehabili­ tation. as a starting point, therapists’ participation in community outreach programmes would be advantageous. conclusion this study found that the highest percentages o f clients at­ tending the slt & audiology opd at a tertiary hospital in the western cape attended for a hearing loss (n=250; 29%) or voice disorder (n=180; 21%) and that the ent department at the study hospital (n=203; 24%) and the education sector (n=l 11; 13%) re­ ferred the highest percentages of clients. in terms of age, the high est percentage of clients attending the opd were between 7 and 12 years old (n=165; 18%). overall there were similar percentages of male and female clients who attended the opd. english speaking clients predominated (n=501; 55%), followed by afrikaans speak­ ing clients (n=298; 33%). only a small percentage of clients were xhosa speaking (n=82; 9%). patterns of attendance showed that almost half of all the clients who attended the opd attended only one appointment (n=353; 49%). in total just over 70% of clients attended either no more than two appointments (n=506; 70%) or attended within a three month period (n=513; 71%). with an in­ creasing number of scheduled appointments, percentage of ap­ pointments attended decreased. given the poor attendance rates and the high number of cli­ ents in the 7 to 12 year age group seen at the slt & audiology opd, there is a definite need for more adequate service provision in schools. the department of education in the western cape has very limited slt & audiology services (f. lewis, personal com­ munication, february 18, 2005). however, the department of education (2001) emphasises the need to optimize the expertise of specialist support personnel, such as therapists, psychologists, re­ medial educators and health professionals within the school set­ ting. in the long term therefore, the service provision for learners in inclusive settings must be given urgent consideration. cur­ rently, the education sector in the western cape does not have the capacity for individual intervention in ordinary classrooms, but plans to provide preventative services and services to educators (f. lewis, personal communication, february 18, 2005). one of the major challenges facing the slt & audiology profession is to provide equitable services for the multilingual populations. the scarcity of xhosa-speaking therapists in the western cape is a source of concern. in the long term it would be advantageous to train and employ professionals speaking african languages. however, given the language diversity in the country, it is critical that practitioners become skilled to manage multilin­ gual and multicultural populations (swanepoel, 2005). for exam­ ple, the inclusion of interpreters to support clinical practice is im­ portant. however, there were no available interpreters at the slt & audiology opd under study, despite repeated requests to the hospital authorities in this regard. while service delivery to multi­ lingual populations remains a challenge, it is important that steps are taken to create equitable practices. references l abramson, j. (1990). survey methods in community medicine. edin­ burgh: churchill livingstone. ! alant, e. (1989). language intervention at schools: changing orientations within the south african context. the south african journal of communication disorders, 36: 9-13. a llied health professionals technical committee (february 2004). reha­ bilitation 2010: draft 1. bhagwanjee, a., & stewart, r. (1999). disability research in south af­ rica: vision and imperatives for a national coordinated approach. south african journal o f occupational therapy, 29(1): 15-17. blake, r. (1981). disabled older persons: a demographic analysis. jour­ nal o f rehabilitation, 47(4), 19-27. broomfield, j., & dodd, b. (2004). children with speech and language disability: caseload characteristics. international journal o f lan­ guage and communication disorders, 39(3): 303-324. d ie suid-afrikaanse tydskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) profiles o f outpatient speech-language therapy and audiology clients at a tertiary hospital in the western cape 57 council for international organisations o f medical sciences [cioms] (1991). international guidelines f o r ethical reviews o f epidemi­ ologic studies. geneva: cioms,. coyle, s., weinrich, b., & stemple, j. (2001). shifts in relative preva­ lence o f laryngeal pathology in a treatment-seeking population. journal o f voice, 15(3): 424-440. das, j. (2001). reading difficulties & dyslexia. n ew jersey: sarka educational resources inc. department o f education (2001). white paper 6: special education: building an inclusive education and training system. pretoria: government printer. edwards, m„ cape, j., & brown, d. (1989). patterns o f referral for children with speech disorders. child: care, health and devel­ opment, 15: 417-424. enderby, p. (1989). the epidem iology o f communication disorders. in m. leahy (ed.), disorders o f communication: the science o f intervention. london: taylor and francis. enderby, p., & davies, p. (1989). communication disorders: planning a service to meet the needs. british journal o f disorders o f communication, 24: 301-331. enderby, p., & petheram, b. (2000). an analysis o f referrals to speech and language therapy in 11 centres, 1987-95. international journal o f language and communication disorders, 35(1): 137-146. farmer, h. (1990). can retrospective research be done from records? s. a. journal o f occupational therapy, 20: 18-21. forman, l., pillay, y., & sait, l. (2004). health legislation 1994 2003. in health systems trust, south african health review 2003/04. durban: health systems trust. guitar, b. (1998). stuttering: an integrated approach to its nature and treatment (second edition). maryland, baltimore: lippincott williams and wilkins. hall, w., haynes, r., & mccoy, d. (2002). the long road to the d is­ trict health system: legislation and structures fo r the dhs in south africa: an appraisal as at august 2002. durban: health systems trust. heron, c / (2001). speech and language therapy for adults in hack­ ney: an assessment o f need. international journal o f commu­ nication disorders, 56(suppl): 46-51. hite, s. (2001). reviewing quantitative research to inform educa­ tional policy processes. paris: unesco: international institute for educational planning. j \ ijumba, p., & day, c. (2004). preface. in health systems trust, south african health review 2003/04.s durban: health systems trust. katzenellenbogen, j., joubert, g., & abdool karim, s. (eds). (1997). epidemiology: a manual fo r south africa. cape town: oxford university press. klop, d. (1998). quality management in a private speech-language therapy practice. unpublished m.sc. (speech and language pathology) thesis, university o f cape town. lubker, b. (1997). epidemiology: an essential science for speech language pathology and audiology. journal o f communication disorders, 30: 251-267. lubker, b., & tomblin, j. (1998). epidemiology: informing clinical practice and research on language disorders o f children. topics in language disorders, 19(1): 1-26. mclaren, p., solarsh, g., & saloojee, g. (2004). disability and disable­ ment. in health systems trust, south african health review 2003/04. durban: health systems trust. ntuli, a., & day, c. (2004). ten years on have w e got what we or­ dered? in health systems trust, south african health review 2003/04. durban: health systems trust. owens, r. (1999). language disorders: a functional approach to assessment and intervention (third edition). boston: allyn and bacon. petheram, b., & enderby, p. (2001). demographic and epidemiological analysis o f patients referred to speech and language therapy at eleven centres 1987-95. international journal o f language and communication disorders, 36(4): 515-525. phillips, j. (2004). language delay: w hy do parents fail appointments? international journal o f therapy and rehabilitation, 11(11): 500. pillay, m., kathard, h., & samuel, m. (1997). the curriculum o f prac­ tice: a conceptual framework for speech-language therapy and audiology practice with a black african first language clientele. the south african journal o f communication disorders, 44: 109-117. raia, p. (1992). what are w e trying to restore? a case for habilitation. generations, 16(1): 37-39. rhoda, a. (2001). a profile o f stroke clients treated a t the bishop lavis rehabilitation centre between 1995-1999. unpublished master of medical sciences (rehabilitation) thesis, university o f stel­ lenbosch. roulstone, s., glogowska, m., peters, t., & enderby, p. (2004). build­ ing good practice: lessons from a multimethod study o f speech and language therapy. international journal o f therapy and rehabilitation, 11(5): 199-204. schneider, m. (1992). the nature and management o f communication disorders in a rural area: the role o f the community speech and hearing therapy workers. south african journal o f communi­ cation disorders, 39: 55-61. south africa info reporter. (2002). s a ’s population by province. re­ trieved february 21, 2005, from http://www.southafrica.info/ ess_info/sa_glance/demographics/popprov.htm statistics south africa (2004). retrieved june 22, 2004, from super c ro ss. s p a c e t im e r e s e a r c h w w w .i n f o .g o v .z a / annualreport/2003/statssa03.pdf statistical package for the social sciences inc. (1995). statistical p ackage f o r the social sciences. chicago, illinois: spss inc. swanepoel, d. (2005). infant hearing screening at m aternal and child health clinics in a developing south african community. university o f pretoria: unpublished dphil dissertation. swartz, l. (1998). culture and mental health: a southern african view. cape town: oxford university press. whitelaw, d., meyer, c., bawa, s., & jennings, k. (1994). post­ discharge follow-up o f stroke patients at groote schuur hospi­ tal a prospective study. south african medical journal, 84:11-13. the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) http://www.southafrica.info/ http://www.info.gov.za/ 58 wendy overett and harsha kathard appendix a data collection form demographic information date in file_ client's n a m e:________________________ date o f birth: folder number:_____________ gender: male female home language:_____________ full residential address: monthly family income: n o o f dependants: hospital category: free ho hi h2 h3 private marital status: single w idowed married divorced n ot given n a occupation: pensioner grant other:________________________________ diagnostic information medical d iagn osis:__________________________________________________ speech/audio diagnosis: referral source: date assessed: attendance information (write every date) dates attended therapy sessions: dates cancelled therapy sessions: dates o f appointments missed: d ie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) article information authors: deidré breytenbach1 alta kritzinger1 maggi soer1 affiliations: 1department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa correspondence to: deidré breytenbach email: deidre1bez@gmail.com postal address: 12 birch avenue clubview, extension 2, pretoria dates: received: 28 nov. 2014 accepted: 30 may 2015 published: 09 dec. 2015 how to cite this article: breytenbach, d., kritzinger, a., & soer, m. (2015). audiology practice management in south africa: what audiologists know and what they should know. south african journal of communication disorders 62(1), art. #114, 9 pages. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v62i1.114 copyright notice: © 2015. the authors. licensee: aosis openjournals. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. audiology practice management in south africa: what audiologists know and what they should know in this original research... open access • abstract • introduction • method    • aim    • objectives    • research design    • ethical considerations    • respondents    • materials    • procedures    • data analysis • results and discussion    • existing knowledge of audiology practice management tasks    • required knowledge to perform audiology practice management tasks    • differences between the existing levels of knowledge and the required levels of knowledge necessary to perform practice management tasks as perceived by the respondents    • comparison between existing and required levels of knowledge for public and private working environments    • strengths, management challenges and training needs as perceived by respondents    • recommendations regarding audiology practice management training • conclusion • acknowledgements    • competing interests    • authors’ contributions • references abstract top ↑ background: in future, the south african department of health aims to purchase services from accredited private service providers. successful private audiology practices can assist to address issues of access, equity and quality of health services. it is not sufficient to be an excellent clinician, since audiology practices are businesses that must also be managed effectively. objective: the objective was to determine the existing and required levels of practice management knowledge as perceived by south african audiologists. method: an electronic descriptive survey was used to investigate audiology practice management amongst south african audiologists. a total of 147 respondents completed the survey. results were analysed by calculating descriptive statistics. the z-proportional test was used to identify significant differences between existing and required levels of practice management knowledge. results: significant differences were found between existing and required levels of knowledge regarding all eight practice management tasks, particularly legal and ethical issues and marketing and accounting. there were small differences in the knowledge required for practice management tasks amongst respondents working in public and private settings. conclusion: irrespective of their work context, respondents showed that they need significant expansion of practice management knowledge in order to be successful, to compete effectively and to make sense of a complex marketplace. introduction top ↑ audiology in south africa has, over the past five decades, advanced from a combined profession of speech and hearing therapy into two interweaved, but autonomous, professions of audiology and speech-language therapy (edwards, 2009; swanepoel, 2006). audiology has now diversified within this multiracial, multilingual, and multicultural context as a hearing healthcare profession aimed at providing quality services to meet the diverse needs of the entire population (swanepoel, 2006). audiologists are offered a wide selection of practice opportunities across a number of work settings in south africa. the country has a large public sector and a smaller but fast-growing private healthcare sector (bakker, 2008). according to lefemine (2012), the private sector in south africa attracts the majority of the country's health professionals. consequently, there is a shortage and maldistribution of key healthcare workers across the rural-urban and public-private divides (swanepoel, 2006; ward, sanders, leng & pollock, 2014). annually, an estimated 6116 infants will be born with or acquire permanent bilateral hearing loss, with approximately 92% born in the public health sector (swanepoel, storbeck & friedland, 2009). therefore, one of the main challenges in the public health care sector is a shortage of audiologists (kanji & kara, 2013). as part of improving the healthcare system and ensuring that all south africans have equitable access to essential healthcare services, the south african government is introducing the national health insurance (nhi) system. in future, the department of health aims to purchase services from accredited private service providers (matsoso & fryatt, 2013; ward et al., 2014). the strategy aims to increase the number of health care personnel indirectly by enjoining those in the private sector to provide services to the general public (george, quinlan, reardron & aguilera, 2012). the success of private audiology practices is, therefore, also important for the future engagement between sectors in order to address issues of access, equity and quality of health services by increasing private sector participation. the future of private audiology practices depends on how well audiologists are able to take their clinical training and practice management skills to the marketplace (metz, 1996). this basic and longstanding requirement also applies to the future of audiology practices in south africa. a successful audiologist must provide exceptional patient care and customer service regardless of the setting, but if equal attention is not paid to the business aspect of the practice the success of the practice could be risked (gnewikow, gnewikow & cieliczka, 2009; hosford-dunn, roeser & valente, 2008). according to clark and benson (2008) audiologists must understand the difficulty of balancing the need to serve the public in the highest ethical manner and also making a living by operating a business. the practice management requirements of the private audiology practice may also differ from the audiologist's patient-first professional motives (hosford-dunn et al., 2008). therefore, practice management is the most underestimated challenge to the private audiologist, since every audiology practice becomes a business that must also be managed effectively (gnewikow et al., 2009; traynor, 2006). although a wide variety of core audiology skills need to be mastered prior to working as an audiologist, practice management proficiency is of equal importance in any environment. audiologists must be able to use many management skills in practice that may not have been acquired in undergraduate training (clark & benson, 2008). there appears to be a lack of recent research regarding practice management needs amongst south african audiologists. in a country-wide south african survey by wemmer (2007) a significant number of respondents (35%) indicated that their undergraduate education left them unprepared for practice management, whilst 22% indicated that it had not been included in the undergraduate curriculum. in another south african study conducted by bakker (2008), 86% of respondents did not receive practice management training in their undergraduate studies. according to a survey of audiologists (n = 256) in the united states of america (usa) and canada (henson, williamson & jacques, 2006) it was clear that practicing audiologists in those countries also felt they did not have the required business skills to compete in the marketplace. respondents reported great deficits in management and business knowledge that impacted their work and careers (henson et al., 2006). it is not currently known how practice management training is perceived by audiologists in south africa. it was therefore important to conduct a survey to determine context specific management challenges, strengths and training needs as well as the self-perceived existing and required levels of practice management knowledge amongst south african audiologists. this survey included eight practice management tasks that may be considered as the most important body of knowledge for business, namely accounting, finance, marketing, legal and ethical issues, organisational behaviour and human resources, operations and systems management, strategic management, and managerial decision-making (henson et al., 2006; henson, presley & korfmann, 2008; hosford-dunn et al., 2008). the results may identify differences between the existing levels of knowledge and the required levels of knowledge of the respondents. this may lead to the formulation of recommendations regarding practice management training for south african audiologists. method top ↑ aim the aim of the study was to determine the self-perceived existing and required levels of practice management knowledge amongst south african audiologists. objectives to determine the existing levels of knowledge necessary to perform practice management tasks as perceived by the respondents. to determine the required levels of knowledge necessary to perform practice management tasks as perceived by the respondents. to determine if there are differences between the existing levels of knowledge and the required levels of knowledge necessary to perform practice management tasks as perceived by the respondents. to determine if there are differences between the existing levels of knowledge and the required levels of knowledge between respondents working in public settings and private settings. to describe strengths, management challenges and training needs as perceived by the respondents. to describe the respondents’ recommendations regarding practice management training for south african audiologists. research design a descriptive survey using electronic questionnaire distribution was used to investigate audiology practice management amongst south african audiologists. a web-based survey was deemed the most effective method to gather the opinions of as many south african audiologists as possible. ethical considerations ethical clearance to conduct the study was obtained from the research ethics committee of the faculty of humanities at the university of pretoria (reference number: 26162289). the data was collected anonymously and treated with confidentiality. no identifying information of the respondents was reported and internet protocol addresses were not tracked. an informed consent letter formed part of the web-based survey and was the first page that respondents viewed once they clicked on the link. by completing the web-based survey respondents gave their consent to participate in the study on a voluntary basis. respondents the population of interest was registered audiologists and dually qualified speech-language therapists and audiologists working in public or private settings in south africa – whether they are involved in practice management or not. a non-probability, convenience sampling strategy was employed. because a large number of audiologists are still dually qualified, it was not possible to determine how many of the professionals are practicing only as audiologists. at the time of data collection there were 1749 audiologists and dually registered speech-language therapists and audiologists registered with the health professions council of south africa (hpcsa, 2013), but email addresses could not be obtained. the hpcsa does not supply practitioner contact information such as email addresses, but postal addresses may be purchased from them (hpcsa, 2013). in addition to their professional registration, some audiologists also joined professional organisations such as the south african association of audiologists (saaa) and the south african speech-language-hearing association (saslha). the email was sent to the databases of saaa (n = 326) and saslha (n = 1300) as their member databases are updated annually, and they were able to send the survey to all their members. since there was no effective way to determine which of the saslha members practice only as speech-language therapists, a number of redundant emails were sent to speech-language therapists. some audiologists are also registered with both the saaa and saslha, meaning that some respondents might have received an invitation to participate twice. an email message providing the web address where the respondent could link directly to the survey was thus sent to a total of approximately 1626 prospective respondents. a total of 147 respondents completed the survey indicating a minimum response rate of 9%. a description of the respondents is given in table 1. table 1: description of respondents (n=147). according to table 1 the respondents consisted mostly of females (97.9%), which is consistent with the population of audiologists in south africa (wemmer, 2007). ages ranged between 23 and 61 years with a mean age of 34.9 years. the majority of respondents were dually qualified as speech-language therapists and audiologists (65.4%) with a bachelor's degree obtained from the university of pretoria (55.1%). the small sample size and the fact that most respondents graduated from one tertiary institution influenced the generalisability of the findings, especially findings regarding training. the largest group of respondents were full time employees (41.8%) working in a private practice (54.6%) located in an urban area (74.3%) in the gauteng province (47.5%). very few respondents worked in a public setting (30.8%) compared to those who worked in a private context (69.2%). it is possible that private practitioners had a greater interest in the topic of practice management than respondents working in a public setting, and therefore more respondents from a private context participated in the study. there is also incongruity regarding the number of respondents employed in each province and the size of each province. some provinces, such as the northern cape, were underrepresented in the survey. there were a few missing values (n = 11) as demographic information was obtained last in the web-based survey, and some respondents did not complete all the questions. materials a web-based survey was developed by consulting previous studies regarding practice management (henson et al., 2006), and refined after a pilot study was completed. structured closed-ended questions and open-ended questions were used in the survey. section a was assigned to training and education in practice management to gather information regarding the level of training respondents have received in practice management. this section requested the respondent's opinions regarding the need for practice management training, and when and how it should be presented. their opinions on what the content, duration, and format of such training should entail was also requested. section b was assigned to management in practice. this section was used to determine the respondents’ challenges as well as strengths in practice management. in this section the researcher also determined the existing and required levels of knowledge necessary to perform practice management tasks as perceived by the respondents. questions in this section were based on a study conducted by henson et al. (2006), which included eight areas which may be considered as the most essential knowledge for business, namely accounting, finance, marketing, legal and ethical issues, organisational behaviour and human resources, operations and systems management, strategic management, and managerial decision-making. section c was assigned to demographic information to gather information about the profile of respondents and their audiology practices. according to haslam and mcgarty (2014), demographic information should be gathered at the end of a survey as respondents tend to lose interest when too much demographic information is asked in the beginning. procedures a pilot study was conducted to pre-test the web-based survey. the preliminary survey was sent to nine respondents. they recommended changes to the flow of the questions and the format of skip-questions to enhance the efficacy and practicality of the survey. for the main study, saaa and saslha sent an email message to their databases. the email message consisted of a cover letter addressed to prospective respondents, inviting them to participate in an anonymous web-based survey. the message also provided the web address to link directly to the survey. surveymonkey was used to collect the data online. two weeks after the first email was sent, a second email was sent reminding prospective respondents to complete the survey and informing them of the closing date. the survey was open for participation for three months as data collection coincided with december 2013 and january 2014 school holidays. data analysis the data from surveymonkey was exported into the statistical package for the social sciences (ibm spss, version 22) for statistical analysis. results were quantitatively analysed by calculating descriptive statistics such as percentages, frequency distribution, measures of central tendency and standard deviation. this assisted in organising and summarising the data. the z-proportional test (maree, 2007) was used to determine if two groups of respondents differed significantly on selected characteristics. content analysis (leedy & ormrod, 2010) was also used to analyse qualitative data derived from open-ended questions. underlying patterns, key themes and trends were identified by thorough and systematic examination of the text data collected from the web-based survey. results and discussion top ↑ existing knowledge of audiology practice management tasks respondents were requested to evaluate their perceived existing levels of knowledge regarding eight practice management tasks and rate them as very low, low, high or very high on a 4-point likert scale. very low and low results and high and very high results were combined to summarise the data. the results are presented in table 2. table 2: existing levels of knowledge (n=147). results in table 2 indicated that respondents’ existing knowledge of practice management tasks were mostly low or very low for all eight practice management tasks. operations and systems management was the practice management task respondents reported they knew the most about (56.1%). operations and systems management is an integral part of an audiologist's daily tasks, which includes managing the processes that produce and distribute products and services such as diagnostic hearing tests and the fitting of hearing aids. respondents may therefore mostly report a high level of existing knowledge regarding operations and systems management. accounting was the task they knew the least about (37.4%). the results were in agreement with henson et al. (2008), who found that accounting was the task that chiropractors in the usa knew the least about. knowledge of accounting is necessary to make financial decisions – from purchasing equipment and supplies to expanding services and determining salaries (traynor, 2008). accounting is a specialised field and would not have been included during undergraduate studies. according to clark and benson (2008) audiologists must have an understanding of basic bookkeeping and accounting. required knowledge to perform audiology practice management tasks respondents were requested to indicate required levels of knowledge regarding eight practice management tasks and rate them as very low, low, high or very high on a 4-point likert scale. very low and low results and high or very high results were combined to summarise the data. the results are presented in table 3. table 3: required levels of knowledge (n=147). results in table 3 indicated that the respondents’ required levels of knowledge regarding practice management tasks were high or very high for all eight business tasks. respondents were aware of the high need for practice management knowledge. respondents were of the opinion that they required the most knowledge about the practice management task of marketing (95.8%). marketing is the creation of demand for a particular product or service by establishing public awareness (taylor, 2015b; traynor, 2006). audiologists have to market their services and qualifications to the community to create greater awareness of the available services, but must also follow the ethical rules regarding advertising (hpcsa, 2008). this is especially applicable to private practitioners, where marketing is integral to their success (taylor, 2015b). therefore, the development of marketing skills should be as much a priority as the development of hearing evaluation skills (kotler & keller, 2009; staab, 2008). finance, accounting and other business functions will be irrelevant in the absence of a sufficient demand for products and services in order to make a profit (kotler & keller, 2009). legal and ethical issues (95.2%) were also rated highly. finance (91.8%), accounting (91.7%) and strategic management (91.7%) were considered by the respondents to be equally important. an ability to understand the financial drivers of a successful practice is a fundamental and long-lasting skill set that will benefit any professional regardless of his or her work setting (traynor, 2008). respondents realised the importance of this by rating the required levels of knowledge highly. organisational behaviour, managerial decision-making, operations and systems management were rated lower in terms of required knowledge. differences between the existing levels of knowledge and the required levels of knowledge necessary to perform practice management tasks as perceived by the respondents by combining the two tables, differences in knowledge can be highlighted. the difference between the required and the existing levels of knowledge levels is indicated in table 4. table 4: difference between respondents’ existing levels of knowledge and required knowledge necessary to perform practice management tasks (n=147). as indicated in table 4 the difference in knowledge is the difference between the percentages of respondents who described their existing and required levels of knowledge as high or very high. this result was not obtained from the respondents directly but rather serves as an informative way to summarise the data and highlight differences in knowledge as previously done by henson et al. (2006). as indicated in table 4 the z-proportional test revealed statistically significant differences between the required and existing levels of knowledge for all eight practice management tasks (p < 0.05). the results are in agreement with henson et al. (2006), who also found a significant difference between required and existing levels of knowledge amongst respondents in the usa and canada. the majority of the respondents (95.2%) in the current study were of the opinion that audiologists need high or very high levels of knowledge about legal and ethical issues to effectively manage audiology practices. in reality only 39.5% were of the opinion that they possessed high or very high knowledge about legal and ethical issues. the results were in agreement with naudé and bornman (2014), who found that despite the fact that knowledge of ethics in audiology grew between 1980 and 2010, retrospective analysis identified gaps in the current knowledge. this was the largest difference amongst the practice management tasks followed, again, by the difference in knowledge about accounting (54.4%) and marketing (52.3%). this disparity may partly be due to a lack of opportunity for audiologists to acquire fundamental knowledge of practice management. according to traynor (2006) it is not surprising that audiologists perform outside their educated expertise in these areas. comparison between existing and required levels of knowledge for public and private working environments table 5 indicates a comparison between the existing and required levels of knowledge amongst respondents working in public or private settings. table 5: comparison of existing and required levels of knowledge between public (n=41) and private (n=92) working environments. according to table 5, there were small differences between existing knowledge in private and public settings. the z-proportional test however, revealed statistically significant differences between existing levels of knowledge regarding marketing (p = 0.001), organisational behaviour and human resources (p = 0.049) and operations and systems management (p = 0.000). respondents working in a private setting had a higher existing knowledge regarding these three tasks. respondents working in a private setting may gain more experience regarding marketing as they have to actively market their practices. it is possible that respondents working in the public sector might be overburdened by the demand for their services and therefore have limited knowledge regarding marketing. respondents working in the private sector are solely responsible for organisational behaviour and human resources. in a public setting these responsibilities are handled collectively. the z-proportional test revealed no significant differences between respondents working in a public and/or private context regarding their required levels of knowledge in all eight of the practice management tasks. the results indicated that there is a great need for knowledge regarding practice management irrespective of the working environment. respondents work in a variety of employment contexts throughout their careers with various common traits; for example, they all have to conform to legal and ethical constraints. therefore, the required level of knowledge regarding legal and ethical issues is high, regardless of the employment context. strengths, management challenges and training needs as perceived by respondents respondents indicated in the text data section that their biggest strengths were patient satisfaction, successful marketing, and starting their own practices. respondents indicated that a lack of training, knowledge, experience and sufficient finances were their biggest challenges in practice management in south africa. in the open comments section of the survey, one respondent stated that ‘a lack of knowledge and education before starting your own private practice is the biggest challenge in practice management in south africa’. the majority of respondents indicated that training is required to overcome these challenges. recommendations regarding audiology practice management training according to table 6, the majority of respondents (80.8%) believed that there is a need for practice management and that such training should be offered at an undergraduate level. taking into consideration that audiologists working in both public and private settings had a low existing level of knowledge, training at an undergraduate level would be ideal. in reality, just under a quarter (22.7%) of the respondents indicated that it was presented as an undergraduate module, emphasising the need for change regarding practice management training. continuing professional development (cpd) activities were also rated highly (57.7%). for audiologists to maintain their registration with the hpcsa they have to obtain 60 cpd points in a two year cycle (hpcsa, 2011). therefore, cpd activities will be a good means to address the need for practice management training and acquiring cpd points. table 6: summary of training recommendations (n=147). when asked who should co-ordinate practice management training, individuals with practice management experience (59.6%), and departments of audiology at universities were rated highly. the latter indicated that respondents wanted to learn from lecturers with experience in the field, and that universities are held in high regard by the respondents. according to fasokun, katahoire and akpovire (2005), experience is regarded as more important than knowledge amongst adults in south africa, therefore respondents may have rated individuals with experience highly. respondents recommended that practice management training should be presented as a short course (45.7%), or as on-the-job training (43.1%), but distance learning was not favoured (4.82%). since respondents were mostly young female professionals in their thirties, employed full-time in private practices (table 1) with little spare time, having to balance both career and family life they may have preferred short courses or in-service training. according to fasokun et al. (2005) adult learners are physically, psychologically and culturally different from young learners. as a result of differences, adults apply habitual styles when learning. owing to their individual needs, adult learners may easily feel left out of learning activities, and this may be why distance learning was not favoured by respondents (baloglu, 2007; fasokun et al., 2005). the majority of respondents (60%) preferred assignments throughout the course. according to gibbs and simpson (2004), adult learners consider coursework to be fairer than exams and measure a greater range of abilities. the quality of learning has also been shown to be higher in assignment-based courses (gibbs & simpson, 2004). marketing was rated as the most important topic to be included in training (93.5%), which is in agreement with the required levels of knowledge as indicated by the respondents in table 3. respondents have to promote their private practices, since these are essentially small businesses. according to staab (2008), audiologists must understand the fundamental principles of marketing and have basic marketing skills. great emphasis was placed on basic marketing skills as marketing the profession is also important for the future of audiology. marketing was closely followed by legal and ethical issues (91.5%). ethical considerations should go hand in hand with promoting a practice (solodar & williams, 2007). audiologists have a professional code of ethics and standards as well as guidelines for good practice (hpcsa, 2008; saslha, 2011) that ensures high ethical standards and which provide the foundation for good customer service. according to taylor (2015a), a trusting relationship with the audiologist is rated highly by patients. for this reason audiologists inherently place a large emphasis on legal and ethical issues. conclusion top ↑ this study found significant differences between respondents’ self-perceived existing and required knowledge in all eight practice management tasks. legal and ethical issues, as well as marketing and accounting, revealed the biggest differences. respondents recognised that they need significant expansion of their practice management knowledge, skills and attitudes in order to be successful irrespective of their work context. the success of private audiology practices is also important for the future engagement between private and public sectors when the national health insurance system is implemented. to address these needs audiology programmes should incorporate aspects of all eight practice management areas that compose what is considered the most important body of knowledge for practice management (henson et al., 2006; hosford-dunn et al., 2008). according to henson et al. (2006), there are many options for audiologists in the usa and canada seeking additional practice management education. this may include web-based learning, professional association conventions, continuing professional education activities, manufacturer support, mentoring, books and educational opportunities outside those tailored to the profession. despite the assistance provided by these individual opportunities, simpson (2011) states that one of the most prevalent means of business education for audiologists remains that of “trial and error”. according to bakker (2008), audiologists receive excellent clinical training, but limited or no formal preparation for the challenges that the management of a private practice brings. audiology practice management is a specialised field as audiologists face unique challenges such as marketing restrictions stipulated by the hpcsa (2008). there are several alternatives that may address this need as perceived by the respondents, such as future research into the content and methods taught in undergraduate training programmes in south africa to make specific recommendations for incorporating additional practice management training, as recommended by respondents. according to henson et al. (2008) giving up clinical modules to practice management modules or extending programme durations will be difficult as the focus of most university programmes internationally is on clinical training, as this is the core professional function. therefore, postgraduate training, continued professional education and short courses as recommended by the respondents, can be considered. henson et al. (2008) recommended an industry-wide effort to develop and manage a practice management education programme designed specifically for healthcare professionals. this effort could be led by a national or international association and developed at universities that offer audiology programmes (henson et al., 2008). taking into consideration what audiologists know and what they should know and using their recommendations to make improvements, practice management training has the potential to enhance all aspects of the profession, improve service delivery, empower practitioners, create awareness of the profession and increase satisfaction of both providers and patients (hosford-dunn et al., 2008). a limitation of the current study is the fact that the results reported on are derived from a small sample of audiologists and dually qualified speech-language therapists and audiologists in south africa. the response rate was below the desirable rate described in the literature (maxwell & satake, 2006). as a result, the findings may be biased and cannot be generalised to the greater population of audiologists. despite these limitations the data was stable as similar findings were reported by other studies (bakker, 2008; henson et al., 2006; wemmer, 2007), and significant conclusions could be drawn about what respondents know and what they should know. recommendations for further studies include that the same study be conducted with speech-language therapists. most of the respondents (65.4%) were dually qualified as speech-language therapists and audiologists. therefore, some of the respondents might still practice as speech-language therapists as well; hence, it is assumed that speech-language therapists would have similar practice management training needs, although this should be investigated further in a separate study. acknowledgements top ↑ competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships which may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions a.k. (university of pretoria) was the supervisor of the research study. m.s. (university of pretoria) was co-supervisor. d.b. (university of pretoria) conducted the research. a.k., m.s., and d.b. compiled the article. references top ↑ bakker, l. (2008). a study to investigate south african audiologists in private practices’ knowledge of strategic planning and comparison of international norms for small business management. unpublished master's dissertation, university of pretoria, pretoria. baloglu, a. (2007). a flexible mobile education system approach. the turkish journal of educational technology, 6(4), 1–12. clark, t.m., & benson, d. (2008). private practice issues. in h. hosford-dunn, r.j. roeser & m. valente (eds.), audiology: practice management. (2nd edn.). (pp. 128–148). new york: thieme. edwards, a.l. (2009). measurement of distortion product otoacoustic emissions in south african gold miners at risk for noise-induced hearing loss. unpublished doctoral thesis, university of witwatersrand, johannesburg. fasokun, t., katahoire, a., & akpovire, b.o. (2005). the psychology of adult learning in south africa. cape town, south africa: pearson education. george, g., quinlan, t., reardon, c., & aguilera, j-f. (2012). where are we short and who are we short of? a review of the human resources for health in south africa. health sa gesondheid, 17(1), 1–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/hsag.v17i1.622 gibbs, g., & simpson, c. (2004). conditions under which assessment supports student's learning. learning and teaching in higher education, 1(1), 3–31. gnewikow, d., gnewikow, d.w., & cieliczka, d.j. (2009). private practice audiology: coming of age. seminars in hearing, 30(2), 129–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0029-1215440 haslam, s.a., & mcgarty, c. (2014). research methods and statistics in psychology. (2nd edn.). los angeles: sage. health professions council of south africa. (2008). guidelines for good practice in the health care professions. retrieved from http://www.hpcsa.co.za/uploads/editor/userfiles/downloads/conduct_ethics/rules/generic_ethical_rules/booklet_2_ generic_ethical_rules_with_anexures.pdf health professions council of south africa. (2011). continuing professional development guidelines for the health care professionals. retrieved from http://www.hpcsa.co.za/content/docs/guidelines_2011.pdf health professions council of south africa. (2013). current registration statistics. personal communication. henson, s., presley, m., & korfmann, s. (2008). business training and education. needs of chiropractors. the journal of chiropractic education, 22(2), 145–151. http://dx.doi.org/10.7899/1042-5055-22.2.145 henson, s., williamson, s., & jacques, p. (2006). business training and education needs of audiology managers. audiology today, 18(2), 49–54. hosford-dunn, h., roeser r.j., & valente, m. (2008). audiology: practice management. (2nd edn.). new york: thieme. kanji, a., & kara, r. (2013). pediatric physicians’ referral of children aged 0–3 years for audiological evaluation in the public health care sector. audiology research, 3(1), 48–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.4081/audiores.2013.e7 kotler, p., & keller, k.l. (2009). marketing management. (13th edn.). london: prentice hall. leedy, p.d., & ormrod, j.e. (2010). practical research: planning and design. (9th edn.). nj: merrill prentice hall. lefemine, a.a. (2012). us and world medical care. usa: xlibris corporation. maree, k. (ed.). (2007). first steps in research. pretoria: van schaik. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.dsr.2006.12.001 matsoso, m.p., & fryatt, r. (2013). national health insurance: the first 16 months. south african medical journal, 103(3), 156–158. maxwell, l., & satake, e. (2006). research and statistical in communication sciences and disorders. (1st edn.). boston, ma: thomson/delmar learning. metz, m. (1996). service delivery issues in private practice. seminars in hearing, 17(3), 283–294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0028-1083058 naudé, a.m., & bornman, j. (2014). a systematic review of ethics knowledge in audiology 1980–2010. american journal of audiology, 23(2), 151–157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/2014_aja-13-0057 simpson, j.m. (2011). teaching audiology practice management. audiology today, 50–55. solodar, h., & williams, k. (2007). ethics and marketing. seminars in hearing, 28(3), 198–205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2007-982901 south african speech-language hearing association. (2011). code of ethics. ethics and standards committee. braamfontein: saslha office. staab, w.j. (2008). marketing principles and application in audiology. in h. hosforddunn, r.j. roeser & m. valente (eds.), audiology: practice management. (2nd edn.). (pp. 78–100). new york: thieme. swanepoel, d. (2006). audiology in south africa. international journal of audiology, 45(5), 262–266. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14992020500485650 swanepoel, d.w., storbeck, c., & friedland, p. (2009). early detection and intervention in south africa. international journal of pediatric otorrhinolaryngology, 73(6), 783–786. taylor, b. (2015a). in-clinic success: using trust to create advocates in a world of sceptics. audiologyonline, article 13178. taylor, b. (2015b). marketing in an audiology practice. san diego, ca: plural publishing. traynor, r.m. (2006). the business basics of audiology. seminars in hearing, 27(1), 1–3. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2006-932120 traynor, r.m. (2008). practice accounting. in h. hosford-dunn, r.j. roeser & m. valente (eds.), audiology: practice management. (2nd edn.). (pp. 305–317). new york: thieme. ward, k., sanders, d., leng, h., & pollock a.m. (2014). assessing equity in the geographical distribution of community pharmacies in south africa in preparation for a national health insurance scheme. bulletin world health organization, 92, 482–489. http://dx.doi.org/10.2471/blt.13.130005 wemmer, k. (2007). educating audiologists in south africa: internationally recognized specialists or locally relevant generalists? unpublished master's dissertation, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. 17 otitis media and language performance in learning disabilities ν g maritz, β log (pretoria) i c uys, d phil (pretoria) β louw, d phil (pretoria) department of speech therapy and audiology university of pretoria abstract this study was designed to determine the influence of otitis media on the language performance of learning-disabled children and nonlearning-disabled children. four experimental groups were used, viz: learning-disabled children, divided into those with and without a history of recurrent otitis media and average academic achievers again divided into those with and without a history of recurrent otitis media. the language performance of each experimental group was determined using the clinical evaluation and language function (wiig and semel, 1980j. the results indicate that recurrent otitis media impaired the receptive and expressive language performance of the subjects used in this study. the impairment of receptive language was greater than for expressive language. implications for diagnosis, treatment and further research are discussed. opsomming hierdie studie is uitgevoer om die invloed van otitis media op die taalvermoens van leergestremde kinders en nie-leergestremde kinders te bepaal. vier eksperimentele groepe is gebruik, nl. leergestremde kinders verdeel in die met en sonder 'n geskiedenis van herhaalde otitis media en gemiddelde akademiese prestasies weer eens verdeel in die met en sonder 'n geskiedenis van herhaalde otitis media. die taalvermoens van elke eksperimentele groep is bepaal met die "clinical evaluation of language function" (wiig en semel, 1980). die resultate het getoon dat herhaalde episodes van otitis media 'n negatiewe invloed het op die respektiewe en ekspressiewe taalvermoens van die proefpersone gebruik in hierdie studie. reseptiewe taalvermoens is meer aangetas as ekspressiewe taalvermoens. implikasies vir diagnose, behandeling en verdere navorsing word bespreek. learning-disabled children exhibit specific communication problems and are at present referred to as "specific language impaired children "i or "language learning impaired children" (wiig and semel, 1982; sak and ruben, 1982). these children have difficulty understanding or using language, spoken or written, which may lead to impaired ability to listen, think, speak, read, write, spell or do mathematical calculations. perceptual handicaps, brain injury, minimal brain dysfunction, dyslexia and development aphasia, also result in learning disabilities (u.s. office of education, in wiig and semel, 1982). while the influence of otitis media on language development has been investigated, (kessler in northern and downs, 1984; hornsby, 1984) its influence on the language development of learning impaired children has, however, been neglected in the literature. 1 in an effort to list the specific language impairments exhibited by learning-disabled children, many authorities are inclined to fragment and oversimplify complex language processes and to disregard the interrelationship between them. although the symptoms, as described in the literature, cannot be regarded as separate entities, the following impairments are mentioned: wallach and butler (1984) state that learning disabled children often have delayed or deviant auditory perception, synthesis, analysis, memory, discrimination and comprehension abilities. difficulty with concept formation and metalinguistic skills, impoverished and delayed vocabulary skills and delayed rather than deviant language form are mentioned by rapin (1979). eisen (1962) and gottlieb, zinkus and thompson (1979) add delayed phonological, morphological, and syntactic abilities, as well as poor divergent and convergent language production abilities. wiig and semel (1982) found that children with learning disabilities experience delays in achieving communication competence and developing mature styles and social register in interpersonal communication. within the framework of language learning disabilities, it is, however, clear that language cannot be separated from learning, aspects such as analysis and synthesis cannot be separated from auditory perception and language form, content and use cannot be separated from metalinguistic skills. with regard to the influence that otitis media has on language development, research findings can be divided into two groups. the first group of researchers found that otitis die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 © sasha 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) \ 18 a g maritz, i c uys and β louw media has a negative influence on language content and form. children with recurrent otitis media were found to have poor auditory perception, decoding and discrimination abilities (eisen, 1962; gottlieb, et al. 1979; hugo, 1985; sak and ruben, 1982). these children also exhibit a higher incidence of delayed conceptualization, attention and soundblending abilities (eisen, 1962; kessler in northern and downs, 1984). according to rapin (1979) these children have impoverished and delayed vocabulary. children with recurrent otitis media were found to have reduced complexity of syntax, and exhibit phonological development delay. learning impaired children are often poor readers according to kessler in northern and downs (1984) and it is important to note that hugo (1985) and rapin (1979) found that children with recurrent otitis media are often low academic achievers at school. the second group of researchers found that children with a history of otitis media, but whose hearing was normal at the time of testing, suffer little if any deficit, at least in terms of vocabulary, language comprehension, syntax and reading level (owrid, 1970). a possible explanation for the above-mentioned contradictory results is that single or infrequent bouts of middle-ear infection do not have a marked adverse effect on language and speech development or education progress (mustain, 1979). paradise (1983) has, however, stated that methodological weaknesses are present in the above-mentioned research. there is uncertainty about the validity of diagnosed otitis media reported as occurring in early life. there is an absence of information concerning the hearing-thresholds accompanying otitis media episodes and some subjects presented with otitis media at the time of testing. further, the matching of subjects is questionable. only a small number of subjects were tested and the tests used had questionable reliability. it is interesting to note that recurrent otitis media may have an influence on neurological development. studies have shown that the first five years are the most important for brain maturation, with particular emphasis on the second year. there is a critical period in neurological development when the brain is more susceptible to language acquisition (vernon and rothstein, 1968). the first four years form this critical period for the acquisition of language (rubin, 1984). webster (1984) found that the brains of animals with obstructed ear canals often do not mature normally. hearing is thus essential for the development of the auditory pathways in the peripheral and central nervous system. this statement is alarming, as recurrent otitis media may thus impair the development of complex auditory functioning, as well as communicative and cognitive abilities (hornsby, 1984). the effect of recurrent otitis media on the language performance of learning-disabled children has been neglected in the literature. literature has shown, (van rensburg, 1981), that learning-impaired children experience a higher incidence of recurrent otitis media than average academic achievers. the purpose of this study is to determine the effect of recurrent otitis media on the language performance of learning-disabled children, with normal hearing. methodology subject selection 352 questionnaires were distributed to 5 english medium primary schools in pretoria which have remedial classes. the questionnaires were completed by the parents of standard two pupils and collected by class teachers. of the questionnaires distributed 82,5% were returned. 15,75% of the returned questionnaires could not be used due to the fact that they were incomplete or spoiled. the effective percentage of questionnaires that could be used was 69,88% and the effective number of potential subjects, 246. the 24 subjects used in this study were selected by using the table of random numbers method where ' 'the purpose of randomness is to permit blind chance to determine the outcome of the selection process" (leedy, 1985, p. 148). six subjects were allocated to each experimental group (illustrated in table 1) with an even distribution of 3 male subjects and 3 female subjects per group. subjects in experimental groups 1 and 3 were also required to satisfy howie's definition in northern and downs (1984, p. 13): "an otitis-pronechild has the condition 6 or more times before the age of 6, or whose initial episode of otitis media was due to pneumococcus and occurred before the age of 1 year." this information was obtained from the questionnaires. the subjects used english as their home language and had average intelligence, as indicated by the school files. they varied in age from 9 years 6 months to 10 years 3 months (the mean being 9 years 10 months). the experimental groups are described in table 1. the learning-disabled children (experimental groups 1 and 2) showed a discrepancy between intellectual ability and academic achievement, as indicated by the school files. the non learning-disabled children (experimental groups 3 and 4) showed a correlation between intellectual ability and academic achievement, as indicated by the school files. table 1: description of experimental groups (n = 24) group 1 (n = 6) group 2 (n = 6) group 3 (n = 6) group. 4 (n = 6) learningdisabled children with a history of otitis media learningdisabled children without a history of otitis media non-learningdisabled children with a history of otitis media non-learningdisabled ' children without a history of otitis media apparatus and material 1. questionnaire a questionnaire was compiled to select and allocate children to the four experimental groups required for the study. this questionnaire was used to determine the prevalence of otitis media among the subjects. the questionnaire was completed by parents. / the use of questionnaires in research have certain limitations. the researcher does not have direct access to the original data and the questionnaire is an impersonal probe (leedy, 1980). the information obtained using questionnaires may thus have questionable reliability. leedy (1985) provides practical guidelines to reduce impersonality and improve reliability. these the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) otitis media and language performance in language disabilities 19 guidelines include the usage of an initial letter, precise and simple language, courtesy, and 'easy to complete' questionnaire, the inclusion of countercheck questions and an offer of results to the respondent. the questionnaire utilized in this study, was compiled using these guidelines. 2. clinical evaluation of language functions — celf (wiig and semel; 1980) the aim of the celf is to provide differentiated measures of selected language functions in the areas of phonology, syntax, semantics, memory and word-finding and retrieval. these measures were designed to probe specific language processing and production abilities of school-age children. the subtests were designed to assist in the identification of children with learning disabilities, to provide a differential diagnosis of language areas involved through selected language probes and finally to identify areas which require language intervention. a summarized overview of the subtests is provided in table 2. the celf does, however, not provide an in-depth assessment of phonology or pragmatics (wiig and semel, 1980). the celf consists of 3 groups of subtests, viz.: language processing, production and supplementary subtests. in this study the supplementary subtests of the celf (which assess phonology) are considered to form part of the language form/production subtests. this decision is supported by bloom and lahey (1978) who state that language form consists of 3 components, viz.: phonology, morphology and syntax. this decision will also facilitate the discussion of relevant related literature. data collection procedure a comparative design was used. a comparison was made between the language performance of the four experimental groups of subjects on the celf (wiig and semel, 1980). the celf norms have been standardised on the population of the united states of america. it was thus necessary to compare the language performance of subjects included in this study and the existing celf norms. the research design consisted of a systematic evaluation of group differences where stimuli were controlled (smit, 1983). the 24 subjects were individually evaluated in their respective home situations. the tester followed the standardised instructions and administration procedures provided in the celf diagnostic examiner's manual (wiig and semel, 1980). data analysis subtests for each of the 24 celf test forms were totalled, by adding the scores obtained for each item. this information was then statistically analysed by using an ibm 370 computer. the sas programme was utilized and procedures employed were the general linear models procedure, the multiple analysis of variance method (with a 5% level of significance) and duncan's multiple range test (ferguson, 1984). these procedures were used to compare the language performance of the four experimental groups. results the results entail an evaluation of the applicability of the celf to the 4 experimental groups, a description of the level of language performance of each experimental group and an evaluation of intergroup tendencies. evaluation of the applicability of the celf to the 4 experimental groups great caution should be exhibited when using the celf to subtests description of each subtest la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 1 word and sentence structure assesses the child's ability to process and interpret selected word and sentence structures la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 2 word classes 1 evaluates the child's ability to perceive relationships between verbal concepts and identify word pairs la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 3 linguistic concepts evaluates the ability to process and interpret oral directions which contain linguistic concepts requiring logical operations la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 4 relationship and ambiguities evaluates the ability to process and interpret logico-grammatical and ambiguous sentences la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 5 oral directions j evaluates the accuracy, fluency, and speed in naming colours, forms, and colour-form combinations in a sustained confrontation-naming task la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 6 spoken paragraphs! evaluates the ability to process and interpret spoken paragraphs and recall salient information presented la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 7 word series assesses the accuracy, fluency and speed in recalling and producing selected automaticsequential word series la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 8 names on confrontation evaluates the accuracy, fluency, and speed in naming colours, forms, and colour-form combinations in a sustained confrontation-naming task la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 9 word associations evaluates the quantity and quality of the retrieval of semantically related word series from long-term memory la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 10 model sentences assesses productive control of sentence structure in a sentence repetition task la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 11 formulated sentences evaluates the ability to formulate and produce sentences when word and sentence form choices are limited and when semantic and syntactic constraints are introduced by a word which has to be included la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 12 processing speech sounds evaluates the ability to discriminate between speech sounds (phonemes) in minimally different word pairs la n g u a g e c o n te n t / p r o c es si n g s u bt es t s \ 13 producing speech sounds evaluates the accuracy in articulating selected elicited speech sounds (phonemes). table 2: summarized overview of the subtests of the celf |wiig, e. and semel, e., 1980) die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 a g maritz, i c uys and β louw evaluate the language performance of south african subjects. it is important to realize that the celf norms are not necessarily representative of the south african population of school-age children. the celf might for example be unable to identify learning-disabled children from average academic achievers, within the south african population. furthermore, the celf might identify non-existent problem areas in language or may be unable to identify these areas which will have a detrimental effect on the treatment and therapy process. the language performance of experimental groups 1 and 2 (learning-impaired children) was significantly lower than the celf norms. the differences between the language performance of experimental groups 3 and 4 (average academic achievers) and the celf were not significant. a description and comparison of the language performance of experimental groups 1 and 2 on the celf the average performance or arithmetic mean of experimental groups 1 and 2 for each subject, together with the celf norms are listed in table 2. table 3 indicates the celf subtests for which the performance of experimental group 1 was significantly lower (at the 5% level of significance) than experimental group 2. celf subtests standardised celf norms usa population average performance of arithmetic mean of each experimental group (standard deviation in brackets) celf subtests standardised celf norms usa population group 1 group 2 group 3 group 4 1 word and sentence structure (2,30) (2,88) (3,10) (2,94) 38 22* 34,6* 40 36* 2 word classes (2,50) (2,68) (2,88) (2,94) 30 26* 30 34,6 36 3 linguistic concepts (2,40) (2,73) (3,06) (3,17) 34 24* 31* 39 42 4 relationship and ambiquities (2,30) (2,73) (3,14) (3,39) 36 22* 31* 41 48 5 oral directions (2,40) (2,23) (3,18) (3,06) 32 24* 25* 40 39 6 spoken paragraphs (1,00) (1,30) (1,37) (•1,77) 8 4* 7* 7,8* 13 7 word series: item 1 accuracy (1,10) (1,30) (1,30) (1,30) 7 5* 7 7 7 time for item 1 (2,19) (2,14) (1,50) (1,20) 10 20 19 9* 6* word series: item 2 accuracy (1,00) (1,20) (1,70) (1,70) 12 4* 6* 12 12 time for item 2 (2,58) (2,73) (2,14) (1,55) 20 27,6 31 19* 10* 1 8 confrontation naming (2,45) (2,37) (2,91) (2,86)i 32 25* 23* 35 34 time (4,40) (4,94) (4,96) (4,90) ! 100 80,6* 110,5 112,5 . 100 ] 9 word associations (1,55) (1,64) (2,50) (2,45) 20 10* 11* 26 25 10 model sentences (2,19) (2,59) (3,50) (3,79) ι 36 20* 28* 51 60 11 formulated sentences (2,35) (2,64) (2,98) (3,06) 25 23* 29 37 39 12 processing (5,09) (5,25) (5,30) (5,25), 115 118 115 117 115 13 producing speech sounds — blends (3,53) (3,53) (3,35) (3,53) 50 52 52 50 / 52 producing speech sounds — final position (2,08) (2,08) (2,08) (2,08) 18 18 18 18 18 production speech sounds — initial position (2,19) (2,19) ('2,19) (2,19) 20 20 20 20 20 table 3: a summary of the celf norms and the performance of the 4 experimental groups on each celf subtest key: * language performance of that specific experimental group was lower than the celf norm for the subtest standard deviation. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) otitis media and language performance in language disabilities 21 a description and comparison of the language performance of experimental groups 3 and 4 on the celf the average performance or arithmetic mean of experimental groups 3 and 4 for each subtest, together with the celf norms are listed in table 2. table 4 indicates the celf subtests for which the performance of experimental group 3 was significantly lower (at the 5% level of significance) than experimental group 4. table 4: the comparison of the language performance of experimental groups 1 and 2 table 5: the comparison of the language performance celf subtests comparison of the language performance of experimental groups 1 and 2 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 word and sentence structure word classes linguistic concepts relationship and ambiguities oral directions spoken paragraphs word series: item 1 accuracy time for item 1 word series: item 2 accuracy time for item 2 8 confrontation naming time 9 word associations 10 model sentences 11 formulated sentences 12 processing speech sounds 13 producing speech sounds — blends producing speech sounds — final position producing speech sounds — initial position ' key: '* performance of group 1 (at the 5% level of significance) was significantly lower than group 2 for the specific subtest. discussion the celf norms were standardised on a usa population of school-age children. the evaluation of the applicability of the celf to the south african population, has indicated that the celf is applicable to experimental groups 3 and 4, and thus the average academic achievers in this study. it is important to realize the the celf was standardised by using a group of 100 subjects, while experimental groups 3 and 4 consisted of 6 subjects each. the sample population used in this study might not reflect the characteristics resident in the general population accurately. this conclusion is supported by leedy (1985), who states that the larger the sample, the higher the degree to which the sample population will approximate the qualities and characteristics resident in the general population. the preceding discussion has indicated that the celf (wiig and semel, 1980) appears to be a reliable instrument for the identification of learningdisabled children within the south african population. the celf norms were also found to be representative of the average academic achievers in this study. comparison of the language celf subtests performance of experimental groups 3 and 4 1 word and sentence structure 2 word classes 3 linguistic concepts 4 relationship and ambiguities * 5 oral directions 6 spoken paragraphs * 7 word series: item 1 accuracy * time for item 1 word series: item 2 accuracy time for item 2 8 confrontation naming time 9 word associations 10 model sentences * 11 formulated sentences * 12 processing speech sounds 13 producing speech sounds — blends producing speech sounds — final position producing speech sounds — initial position key: * performance of group 3 (at the 5% level of significance) was significantly lower than group 4 for the specific subtest. the inter-group tendencies of the four experimental groups will now be discussed, but as the purpose of the study was not to give an in-depth identification of individual characteristics, this will not be included in this discussion. it is interesting to note that the language content/processing subtests on which experimental groups 3 and 4 differed, also occurred in those subtests for which experimental groups 1 and 2 differed. experimental groups 1 and 3 thus exhibited lower levels of language performance than experimental groups 2 and 4 for the celf language content/processing subtests. experimental groups 1 and 3 consisted of subjects with a history of recurrent otitis media, implying the presence of the fluctuating hearing loss. the presence of a fluctuating hearing loss does not permit the subject with recurrent otitis media to adapt to constant auditory perceptions. this in turn inhibits the development of normal perceptual abilities and may influence language development negatively (kessler, in northern & downs, 1984). the difference exhibited by experimental groups 3 and 4 for the language content/processing subtests was not as great as the difference exhibited by experimental groups 1 and 2. this can be attributed to the fact that experimental group 3 consisted of subjects with average academic achievement. another reason may be that learning-disabled children may be genetically more susceptible to language and learning problems (teele, klein & rosner, 1980). this factor, together with a history of otitis media, may have a more severe influence on language performance. downs (1977) states that recurrent otitis media may result in a syndrome referred to as "irreversible-auditory-learningdie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 a g maritz, i c uys and β louw disaster" and emphasizes that language and learning skills, especially those based on auditory functions are irreversibly damaged by otitis media. the question that poses itself is whether learning-disabled children have a higher incidence of otitis media than children with average academic achievement. research performed by van rensburg (1981) indicated that learning-disabled children have a remarkably higher incidence of otitis media than children with average academic achievement. further research is, however, necessary to confirm this statement. it is interesting to note that tlje language form/production subtests on which experimental groups 1 and 2 differed are the same as on subtests on which experimental groups 3 and 4 differed. experimental groups 1 and 3 exhibited lower levels of language performance than experimental groups 2 and 4 for the celf language form/production subtests. recurrent otitis media appears to exert a negative influence on language form in experimental groups 1 and 3. language form is based on, and also develops simultaneously with language content (bloom and lahey, 1978). a deficit, deviancy or delay in language content will thus influence the development of language form. in experimental groups 1 and 3 (subjects with a history of otitis media) the presence of a fluctuating hearing loss due to recurrent episodes of otitis media had a marked negative influence on the language form of experimental groups 1 and 3. bloom and lahey (1978, pp. 23) state that "children learn language as they use language both to produce and understand messages." language form problems, caused by recurrent otitis media are usually subtle and are difficult to identify unless the child is tested formally (bloom and lahey, 1978; rapin, 1979). this factor may account for the fact that the language form subtests, for which experimental groups 1 and 2 differed, are identically reflected in the results of experimental groups 3 and 4. the celf (wiig and semel, 1982) might not be an instrument that is sensitive enough to identify differences between the experimental groups in this study. according to wiig and semel (1982) the results of the celf should be complemented by the administration of standardized measures of receptive vocabulary development and an analysis of the spontaneous speech sample. the results of this study indicate that recurrent otitis media has a negative influence on language content and form, as has been demonstrated by experimental groups 1 and 3. the influence of recurrent otitis media on the language performance of learning-disabled children has not been evaluated previously. although the size of the population tested in this study was limited, the results indicate that recurrent otitis media may have an adverse effect on the language performance of learning-disabled children. the sample population used in this study might not reflect the characteristics resident in the general population accurately, as the larger the sample, the higher the degree to which the sample population will approximate the qualities and characteristics resident in the general population. the results of this study are important from a clinical point of view. it is essential that speech pathologists and audiologists realise the implications that recurrent otitis media may have on the language performance of learning-disabled children, as well as children with average academic achievement. extensive benefits would be gained if recurrences of otitis media could be prevented or substantially reduced in frequency (northern and downs, 1984). paradise (1980) has suggested five different ways in which the incidence of otitis media episodes may be reduced, viz. adenoidectomy with or without tonsillectomy; antimicrobial prophylaxis; the use of tympanostomy tubes, the frequent and liberal use of myringotomies; and polyvalent pneumococcal vaccine. current literature and the results of this study stress that the clinician should obtain information concerning the occurrence and incidence of otitis media for all children evaluated. this will enable the speech therapist/audiologist to identify at risk children and also extend existing knowledge, concerning the influence that otitis media has on language performance. this study represents the implementation of an exciting recent research direction that should be explored. recommendations for further research are: — to conduct a similar study, using a larger sample, in order to reflect the characteristics of the general population more accurately. — to determine whether children with certain craniofacial characteristics may make them more prone to recurrent otitis media. sharon (1985) suspects that these children have comparatively elongated faces, similar to the findings reported by bresolin, shapiro and shapiro (1984) in children with chronic nasal construction, who breathe through their mouths. — the standardisation of the celf on different language groups in south africa. conclusion this study indicates that the celf appears to be applicable to the south african population of english-speaking schoolage children. the celf also appears to be a reliable instrument for the identification of learning-impaired children within the south african population. further research; is, however, necessary in order to standardize this test for the south african population. recurrent otitis media appears to have a negative influence on the language content and form of learning-disabled children and children with average academic achievement. further research with a larger sample population iis necessary to confirm these research findings. references bloom, l. and lahey, m. language development and language disorders. new york: wiley, 1978. bresolin, d., shapiro, g.g. and shapiro, p.a. facial characteristics of children who breathe through the mouth. pediatrics, 73, 6 2 2 6 2 5 , 1984. / downs, m.p. the expanding imperatives of early identification in bess, f.h. (ed): childhood deafness: causation, assessment and management. new york: grune and stratton, 1977 eisen, n.h. some effects of early deprivation on later behaviour: the quandom hard of hearing child. journal of abnormal social psychology, 65, 338,1962. ferguson, g.a. statistical analysis in psychology and education. singapore: mcgraw-hill, inc., 1984. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) otitis media and language performance in language disabilities 23 gottlieb, m.e. zinkus, p.w. and thompson, m.a., chronic middle -ear disease and auditory perceptual deficits. clinical pediatrics, 18 (12), 7 2 5 7 3 2 , 1979. hornsby, b. overcoming dyslexia. kenwyn: juta and co., ltd., 1984. hugo, r. normatiewe evaluasies van otitis media as sekondere patologie. opvoeding (suid-afrikaanse tydskrif|, 5(2), 83—88, 1985. leedy, p.d. practical research planning and design. new york: mac-millan publishing company, 1985. mustain, w.d. linguistic and educational implications of recurrent otitis media. ear, nose and throat journal, 58, 62—68, 1979. northern, j.l. and downs, m.p. hearing in children. baltimore, maryland: the williams and wilkins, co., 1984. owrid, h.l. hearing impairment and verbal attainment in primary school children. education research, 12, 209—214, 1970. paradise, j.l. otitis media in infants and children. pediatrics, 65, 9 1 7 9 4 3 , 1980. paradise, j.l. long term effects of short term hearing loss — menace or myth? pediatrics, 71, 647—648, 1983. rapin, i. conductive hearing loss. effects on children's language and scholastic skills. a review of literature. annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology, 88, (supplement 60), 3 1 2 , 1979. rubin, r.j. the effects of recurrent middle ear effusion in preschool years on language and learning. audiology in practice, 1 (supplement 3), 5 7 , 1984. sak, r.j. and rubin, r.j. effects of recurrent middle ear effusion in pre-school years on language and learning. journal of developmental and behavioural pediatrics, 3, 7—77, 1982. sharon, b.j. the face in otitis media. pediatrics, 75, 131, 1985. smit, g.j. navorsingsmetodes in gedragswetenskappe. pretoria: haum opvoedkundige uitgewers, 1983. teele, d.w. klein, j.o. and rosner, b.a. epidemiology of otitis media in children. annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology, 89, 5—6, 1980. van resnburg, y.j. chroniese otitis media by die kind met leerprobleme. voorkoms en gevolge. unpublished research study (b.log). university of pretoria, 1981. vernon, m. and rothstein, p. prelingual deafness. archives of general psychiatry, 19, 3 6 1 3 6 7 , 1968. wallach, g.d. and butler, k.g. language learning diabilities in school-age children. london: williams and wilkins, co., 1984. webster, d.b. the effects of recurrent middle-ear effusion in preschool years on language and learning. audiology in practice, 1 (supplement 3), 2, 1984. wiig, e.h. and semel, e.m. clinical evaluation of language function. columbus, ohio: charles e. merrill publishing company, 1980. wiig, e.h. and semel, e.m. language assessment and learning disabled. toronto: merrill publishing company, 1982. -2play [ τ ί and l u schoolroom have moved to bigger a n d better premises at shop 6l the rosebank mews 173 oxford road rosebank jhb. play & schoolroom, specialists in the field of child education have been offering assistance to both professionals and parents for nearly thirty years. their expertise and advice range through pre-school education, perceptual training, primary and remedial education and adult education. play and schoolroom are sole agents for learning development aids which includes an excellent selection of materials of interest to the speech therapist. they also offer an interesting range of aids and books to foster and develop language and communicative skills. their stock of educational books and toys is exceptionally wide. you are invited to view their superb range in their new beautifully laid out showroom. phone 788-1304 po box 52137 (as before) saxonworld 2132 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 35, 1988 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a legacy of sound advice and equipment philips hearing aids audiometers fm systems head office: 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street p.o. box 3069, johannesburg 2000. tel: (011) 337-7537. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 49 the communicative and cognitive deficits following closed-head injury lesley irvine, ba (sp. & h. therapy) (witwatersrand) marlene behrmann, ma (speech path.) (witwatersrand) department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. the communicative and cognitive deficits of three closed-head injured patients were investigated within the framework of language form, language use and cognitive ability. a battery of tests was administered and results indicated a degree of cognitive impairment in all subjects. the balance between the language form and language use skills was subject-specific. these findings supported the current view of heterogeneity within the closed-head injured population as well as the view that a language disorder may result from an underlying cognitive deficit. the results are discussed in the light of the existing literature on head injury. the theoretical and clinical implications are considered. opsomming die kommunikatiewe and kognitiewe vermoens van drie geslotehoofl>eseringspasiente is ondersoek binne die raamwerk van taalvorm, taalgebruik en kognisie. 'n toetsbattery is gebruik en volgens die resultate was daar 'n mate van kognitiewe en kommunikatiewe aantasting in al drie proefpersone. die verhouding tussen taalvorm en gebruiksvaardighede was proefpersoonspesiflek. hierdie bevindinge ondersteun die huidige sienswyse van heterogeniteit binne die gesiotehoofi>eseringspopulasie sowel as die opvatting dat 'n taalafwyking die gevolg van 'n onderliggende kognitiewe afwyking kan wees. die resultate word teen die agtergrond van bestaande literatuur oor geslotehoofbesering bespreek. daar is ook op beide die teoretiese and kliniese implikasies ingegaan. closed-head injury (chi) has recently been termed "the invisible epidemic" (holland, 1982) in view of the well documented dramatic increase in the incidence of this disorder (annegers, grabow, kurland and laws, 1980). the primary source of the pathophysiology in chi is that of blunt trauma to the brain which results in discontinuation of neural substance and shearing and straining of the axons in the white brain matter (hagen 1984). while there seems to be agreement concerning the neurological symptoms of this disorder, considerable controversy still surrounds the neurobehavioural and linguistic sequelae. hagen (1984) has called for controlled, systematic research into the communicative and cognitive deficits which follow chi in order to clarify the existing confusion. from a clinical point of view, the need for further research is pressing in view of the fact that decisions regarding management of chi individuals must be made;, i / j the language symptomatology resulting from chi constitutes an area of much controversy. some writers have considered the language disturbance to be an aphasia (luria, 1970) while others have argued that the language djsorder is unique and clearly distinguishable from classical aphasia (holland, 1982). sarno (1980, 1984) has identified three subtypes of chi subjects, each subtype demonstrating some degree ofllanguage impairment. the one subtype exhibits the traditional aphasic symptoms whilst the remaining two subgroups display subclinical aphasia or a language processing problem without the overt manifestation of classical aphasia. according to sarno (1980; 1984), the typical features of all chi subjects include reduced word fluency, impaired comprehension of complex oral commands and an anomia. it has been suggested that such subtle language problems may only be detected through the use of sophisticated neurolinguistic assessment procedures. additionally, the subtype differences and heterogeneity inherent in the chi population may only be identified on such measures (sarno, 1980). the heterogeneity of these chi subjects is increasingly well recognised and samo's (1980; 1984) classification provides further evidence for the fact that there is no one type of language disturbance resulting from closed-head injury. hagen (1984) has viewed the language problems of the closeddie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 head injured from a very different perspective to that of sarno (1980; 1984). he has argued that chi is characterised by 'confused' language or "a receptive/expressive language that may be intact phonologically, semantically and syntactically yet is lacking in meaning because the behavioural responses are irrelevant, confabulatory, circumventory and tangential" (hagen, 1984: 246). it appears from this definition that the language disorder extends beyond a problem of language form or content, and that the disturbance may be reflected in another area such as that of language function or use. milton, prutting and binder (1984) have supported this view and have stated that the chi subject is communicatively incompetent since language function is typically impaired in this population. they have adopted the premise that language may be divided into the components of form or structure, content and usage. through adopting a more holistic view of language as a component of communication, they have identified the use of language in social context as the major area of deficit in subjects with closed-head injury. this broader communication problem of the chi population has been attributed to one or more of the following factors — psychiatric disturbances, memory problems or a pervasive cognitive deficiency (russell, 1971; holland, 1982). ylvisakerand holland (1985) have recently suggested that the language disruption is secondary to a deficit in conceptual or cognitive processing. while previous research has recognised the existence of cognitive problems such as poor abstract thought and slow speed of processing in chi subjects (hagen, 1984), the contention of a causal relationship is novel. the nature of the relationship between language disturbance and the cognitive deficit remains unresolved. it is, however, a critical issue in terms of rehabilitation of these subjects. the purpose of this study therefore is to examine the language disorder resulting from closed head-injury in relation to the holistic concept of communicative competence as well as in relation to cognitive functioning. it is submitted that a description of the language disturbance from a broader perspective is valuable from a theoretical perspective. furthermore, the clearer definition of the nature of the disturbance should be of assistance in the future management of the chi individual. © sasha 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 lesley irvine and marlene behrmann method subjects three english-speaking adult males who had sustained closedhead injuries as a result of motor vehicle accidents were selected as subjects for this study. the ages of the subjects ranged between 24 to 27,4 years (mean 25,2 years). mean coma duration of the subjects was 11 weeks (range 3-21 weeks), thus indicating a severe head injury according to the criteria prescribed by russell (1971). all subjects were neurologically stable at the time of testing with a mean post-injury time of 39 months (range 15-52 testing as there are no language tests currently available for the specific assessment of chi, it was necessary to utilise and modify tests designed for other disorders such as aphasia. the following tests were administered in order to evaluate the communicative and cognitive functioning of the subjects: a. language form testing — this involved the administration of the boston diagnostic aphasia examination (goodglass and kaplan, 1972) which was utilised to provide an overall account of the language pattern of the subjects. more specific receptive language testing comprised the administration of the revised token test (mcneill and prescott, 1978) and a linguistic test designed to measure performance on complex sentences (penn, 1972). this latter test is designed to assess competence with respect to complex sentences such as relative and complement constructions. the subject was required to answer questions about each of the twenty seven sentences presented to him. sentences were analysed by drawing the deep syntactic structure of the sentence and by observing at what point the subject failed. expressive language testing involved the syntactic analysis of the subjects' output using the language assessment remediation screening procedure (larsp) (crystal, 1982). the data for the months). subjects were all pretraumatically right handed and had completed at least eleven years of schooling. family reports indicated that subjects were of average intellectual capacity prior to the accident. no previous history of sensory or motor deficit was reported nor was any history of drug or alcohol abuse noted. hearing was within normal limits for all three subjects at the time of testing. further relevant clinical and biographical details are included in table 1 below. analysis consisted of a spontaneous interactive language sample which was elicited by one of the authors and simultaneously videotaped and audiotaped through a one-way mirror set-up. topics such as the subject's accident, his family structure, and his hobbies and interests were discussed during this conversational interchange. the sample was then transcribed according to the suggestions of crystal, fletcher and garman (1976) and profiles were completed for each subject. further expressive language testing involved the administration of the expressive component of the linguistic test designed to assess the subject's performance on complex sentences (penn, 1972). the subject was required to repeat twenty five complex sentences and responses were analysed both qualitatively and quantitatively. b. language use testing consisted of the administration of the communicative abilities of daily living (cadl) (holland, 1980). in addition, the interactive sample was analysed on a protocol of communicative skills, the profile of communicative appropriateness (penn, 1983). this profile rates the subject's responses to the interlocutor, control of semantic content, cohesion, fluency, sociolinguistic sensitivity and non-verbal communication. (see table 2 for example of this profile.) the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 table 1: relevant clinical and biographical details for all subjects age subject 1 24 subject 2 24.1 subject 3 27.4 sex male male male premorbid laterality right right right educational level standard 9 apprentice mechanic matric diploma in electronics time since injury to testing (months) 15 52 50 coma length (weeks) 3 10 21 present motor problems l arm hemiplegia bilateral paresis of both arms bilateral extreme spasticity occupation unemployed unemployed unemployed receiving speech therapy yes, since may 1983 no therapy presently yes, since november 1980 immediate post-trauma cat scan results diffuse cerebral oedema with small parietotemporal bleed on left. diffuse cerebral oedema and high intracranial pressure. extensive cerebral involvement including brain stem and right cortical areas. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e communicative and cognitive deficits following closed-head injury table 2: profile of communicative appropriateness (penn, 1983) features of sampling. name unit of analysis person eliciting sample i ii iii iv v vi comments request 2 s reply 1 s clarification request 1-8 \ pi .£ acknowledgement 1-8 \ pi .£ teaching probe others topic initiation topic adherence "s.atopic shift 2 i | lexical choice s ε § o u ο idea completion idea sequencing others ellipsis tense use reference c oh es io r lexical substitution forms c oh es io r relative clauses c oh es io r prenominal adjectives c oh es io r conjunctions c oh es io r others fl ue nc y interjections fl ue nc y repetitions fl ue nc y revisions fl ue nc y incomplete phrases fl ue nc y false starts fl ue nc y pauses fl ue nc y word-finding difficulties fl ue nc y others \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty polite forms , \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty reference to interlocutor \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty placeholders, fillers,[stereotypes \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty acknowledgements | \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty self correction | \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty comment clauses j \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty sarcasm/humour ; \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty control of direct speech \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty indirect speech acts! \ so ci ol in gu is tic se ns iti vi ty others , / n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n vocal aspects: intensity n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n pitch n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n rate n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n intonation n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n quality n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n nonverbal aspects: facial expression n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n head movement n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n body posture n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n breathing n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n social distance n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n gesture and pantomime n on -v er ba l co m m un ic at io n others totals key* i = inappropriate; ii = mostly appropriate; iii = some appropriate; iv = mostly appropriate; v = appropriate; vi = one. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 lesley irvine and marlene behrmann the ratings on this profile were completed by two trained speech and language pathologists who observed the videotapes independently and then completed the score sheet according to uieir observation of the relevant dimensions. both raters had been trained in the use of the profile with the inter-rater reliability being calculated at 0.92 (using the pearson product moment correlation) and hence significant beyond the 0.05 level of significance. communicative competence was analysed quantitatively by computing the overall ratings of appropriate and inappropriate behaviours. c. cognitive testing consisted of the administration of the south african wechsler adult intelligence scale — s.a. wais (national institute of personnel research 1969). scoring was completed according to the prescribed procedure. testing was carried out individually and extended over a number of morning sessions in an attempt to eliminate fatigue as a confounding variable. in order to reduce anxiety, rapport was established with each subject prior to his being tested. results the overall results across the language tests are presented in table 3. in order to allow for comparisons between very different tests and to obtain an overall view of the subjects' performance, the results are presented qualitatively. thus, performance is classified as being either good, mediocre or poor. exhibit a recognisable aphasic profile but problems in both comprehension and expression were observed, a common finding of chi (levin, 1981). the results of the receptive language tests revealed a variation in performance across subjects but consistency across tests. on both the token test (mcneil and prescott, 1978) and the receptive section of the linguistic test for complex sentences (penn, 1972), a severe impairment was noted for si (scores 8 and 45% on the two tests), a moderate receptive language deficit was recorded for s3 (scores 13 and 80,5%) and almost intact language reception was noted for s2 (scores 15 and 96%). holland (1982) has stated that language comprehension problems may persist well into the recovery phase following chi and that comprehension breaks down as the complexity of the material increases. a qualitative analysis of the results of the present study revealed that no one sentence type or grammatical construction presented more difficulty than any other, although increased sentence length was more problematic for all subjects. this may have resulted from short term memory problems, a recognised concomitant of chi (hagen, 1984). table 4 below sets out the major results of the larsp analyses (crystal 1982) for all subjects. table 4: results obtained by all subjected on the revised larsp profile (crystal 1982). table 3: results of subjects' performance on receptive and expressive language testing test results bdae subject 1 subject 2 subject 3 bdae — + + revised token test — + + complex sentences — comprehension + + spontaneous speech — larsp profile + + complex sentences — expression _ + + key: + = good performance ± = mediocre performance — = poor performance a. language form testing the profiles obtained for the subjects on the bd ae were atypical of the aphasic syndrome profiles described by goodglass and kaplan (1972). subjects 2 and 3 (ss2 and 3) were credited with a severity rating of 4 indicating some loss of facility in speech and comprehension. s2 and s3 performed similarly on the various subtests. they showed above mean performance on all subtests with the exception of the verbal agility, animal naming and writing mechanics sections. it is possible that the poor verbal agility score is attributable to the dysarthria which both subjects demonstrated. levin (1981) has observed that a dysarthria frequently accompanies language disturbance in the early stages of recovery post-chi and may persist following language restoration. subject 1 was given a severity rating of 2, suggesting that conversation about familiar subjects is possible although many si s2 s3 total number sentences 313 142 142 mean number sentences per turn 4,6 7,9 2,2 mean sentence length 4,17 10 7,4 total spontaneous utterances 174 125 80 number of clauses 198 212 152 number of phrases 551 698 465 number of word endings 263 276 205 as is evident from this table, ss 2 and 3 presented with well preserved expressive syntactic skills. they demonstrated retention of all constructions with equal balance between phrase, word and clause levels. both these subjects exhibited a high frequency of stage v utterances as well as utterances at stages vi and vii. few (—) features were marked and both demonstrated a high frequency of spontaneous utterances. sections β and d of the profile revealed less interaction with the conversational partner for s2 than for s3. j si presented with a definite expressive syntactic breakdown as evidenced by his larsp sample. he exhibited a high percentage of problematic and unanalysed utterances, a shorter mean sentence length than the other subjects and a high proportion of utterances around stage v with few utterances beyond this stage. many (—) features were marked including omissions and concord errors. the results of the expressive section of the linguistic test of complex sentences supported the above findings. subjects 2 and 3 scoring 80% and 76% respectively, while si scored 0%. si was often unable to recall linguistic elements. in addition, he tended to produce the incorrect word order of the sentence. it is possible, however, that his poor performance resulted from his inability to cope with the memory demands of the task. thus his langauge impairment may be secondary to cognitive disorganisation — an acknowledged pattern of breakdown (hagen, 1984; ylvisaker and holland, 1985). / the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the communicative and cognitive deficits following closed-h b. language use testing the results of the cadl (holland, 1980) revealed relatively well p r e s e r v e d functional skills for all three subjects. this finding was u n e x p e c t e d in view of the results of milton et al. (1984) who proposed that chi leads to an impairment in pragmatic skills. however, these writers used a societal rather than a clinical profile and this renders comparisons impossible. percentage values were computed from the raw scores of the injury cadl. si scored 96% followed by s2 who scored 94% and finally, s3 who obtained 86%. depressed scores were noted on the divergences items as well as on the humour, metaphor and absurdity items. holland (1982) has stated that these items rely on subtle reception of language. the inability to comprehend such items was noted in all three subjects. the results of the profile of communicative appropriateness (pca) (penn, 1983) supported the findings of the cadl (see figure 1 below). appropriate g mostly £ appropriate co a. some jj inappropriate e & •5 mostly as inappropriate inappropriate key : o si s2 s3 response to interlocutor control of semantic content cohesion fluency sociolinguistic sensitivity non-verbal communication dimensions of pca figure 1: performance of all three subjects on the profile of communicative appropriateness the ratings suggested that all three subjects exhibited relatively well preserved communicative abilities. specific areas of breakdown were noted for all the subjects and although these deficits were not overriding, they were sufficient to penalise the subjects in a conversational context. si was rated as mostly inappropriate on the cohesion and fluency scales, possibly as a result of his anomic and syntactic deficits. he was rated as appropriate on the nonverbal communication scale and mostly appropriate on the response to interlocutor, control of semantic content and sociolinguistic sensitivity scales. s2 demonstrated certain problems in interaction largely owing to dysarthria and apparent insensitivity to the interlocutor. apart from poor non-verbal communication, s3 demonstrated well preserved pragmatic skills. some difficulties were noted with suprasegmental features such as vocal intonation, quality and rate of output. once again, this may be attributed to the presence of a dysarthric component in his speech pattern. the differing patterns of performance and the relative preservation of the most pragmatic skills on the pca is unexpected in view of the recognised broad nature of the language deficit of chi. pragmatic abilities are reportedly depressed in the chi population (holland, 1982) and often serve as the pivotal differential diagnostic feature between chi and aphasia. the above findings indicate that pragmatic skills are not completely preserved in any one subject and that whilst an impairment exists, it is not as severe as has been documented in the literature (holland, 1982; milton et al., 1984). the difference between the results of the present study and previous research may be attributable to the fact that different studies utilise differing profiles for analysis and different contexts for data elicitation. c. cognitive testing results of the cognitive testing indicated that each subject had suffered a certain degree of intellectual impairment although the exact degree was subject-specific. s1 obtained a full scale iq score of 75,5 whereas s2 scored 102,5 and s3 scored 82,5. since all subjects had completed at least 11 years of schooling and their families reported no pretraumatic intellectual deficit, one may assume that the depressed cognitive results are associated with the head injury. these findings are supported by hagen (1984) and by levin, benton and grossman (1982) who stated that intellectual impairments are the major residual feature of chi. si's functioning was borderline and a close correlation was noted between his verbal and performance scores. he performed poorly on those tests which required memory skills for example, the digit span and repetition tests. s2 performed at an average level and there was good agreement between his verbal and performance scores. as with si, s3's functioning was borderline and while his verbal score was average, his performance score was much poorer. he demonstrated impaired functioning on all tasks involving spatial-synthetic and visual-constructive abilities. although ssl and 3 performed comparably on the cognitive testing, their language skills differed markedly. this suggests no direct relationship between language abilities and cognitive performances. in sum, the results of the testing in all three areas revealed different patterns of performance for all three subjects. all subjects presented with a degree of language impairment in both form and use. the nature and degree of the impairments, and the balance between language structure and language use skills was subject-specific. these varying patterns lend support to the notion die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 lesley irvine and marlene behrma of heterogeneity within this population (sarno, 1980: 1984). cognitive impairments were noted in all subjects thus supporting the finding of sarno (1980) that "while aphasia sometimes occurs in chi, the far more predominant language disturbances are those that result from the cognitive impairment itself." discussion si performed poorly across all tasks with the exception of his score on the cadl (holland 1980). his results parallel levin et al.'s (1982) finding that patients with a generalised language impairment present with depressed functioning as noted on both the verbal and performance scores of the wais (nipr 1969). s2 performed well in all the areas tested with the exception of specific problems in interaction such as insensitivity to the interlocutor. these results suggest relatively good preservation of language form, use and cognitive skills. it is hypothesised, however, that the few areas of deficit noted on the language use tasks are sufficient to reduce communicative competence and to impede the free flow of conversation. s3 presented with an inconsistent pattern in that he performed relatively well in the areas of language expression and pragmatic skills but exhibited some problems in subtle reception and in cognitive processing. there are several possible explanations for this variation in performance. coma duration and neurological dynamics could possibly account for the differences, as each subject presented different types of brain damage. other variables which may play a significant role in the explanation are non-medical variables such as motivation and personality functioning since affective disorders are a well recognised by-product of chi (hagen, 1984; sarno and levin, 1985). a crucial feature which appears to be operating, however, is the degree of cognitive impairment. this seems to be the main indicator of retention of language skills. it seems from the results that there is a close relationship between the degree of cognitive deficit and the retention of language skills. the variablity of the findings may also be attributed to the fact that the closed-head injured group are a heterogenous group and that individual patterns rather than a single group pattern characterizes this population. this notion is considered in the chi literature (sarno, 1980; sarno and levin, 1985) and is becoming increasingly well accepted. ~~~ a further issue which must be addressed is that of the crucial relationship between language use and language form skills. there does not appear to be a clear dissociation between these as has been suggested in previous studies (milton et al., 1984). this discrepancy gives rise to further debate concerning the applicability of pragmatic competence as a differential diagnostic feature between aphasia and closed-head injury. conclusion the finding that cognitive deficits are a predominant sequela of the closed-head injured population is consistent with the literature (ylvisaker and holland, 1985). the results of this study suggest 'that there is no one type of language disorder associated with chi but that the interaction between language use and language form skills are subject-specific and may be attributed to other factors such as cognitive impairment. the implications of this finding for remediation are many. in view of the heterogeneity, it is essential that the clinician assesses each closed-head injured subject individually without relying on the predicted findings from the literature. furthermore, it is essential to decide whether the cognitive dysfunction should be remediated if it underlies the language disturbance or whether the surface language symptoms should be treated. there are many questions and too few answers at this stage. there is little doubt, however, that chi presents the speech-language pathologist with a "unique and complex diagnostic, prognostic and treatment challenge." (hagen 1984 :245). references annegers, j.f., grabow, j.d., kurland, l.t. and laws, e.r. the incidence, causes and secural trends of head trauma in olmsted county, minnesota. neurology, 30, 912-919, 1980. crystal, d., fletcher, p. and garman, m. the grammatical analysis of language disability: a procedure for assessment and remediation. london: edward arnold, 1976. crystal d. profiling linguistic disability. london: edward arnold, 1982. goodglass, h. and kaplan, e. boston diagnostic aphasia examination. usa: lea and febiger, 1972. hagen, c. language disorders in head trauma. in a. holland (ed.) language disorders in adults. houston: college hill press, 1984. holland, a. communicative abilities of daily living a test of functional communication for aphasic adults. baltimore: university park press, 1980. holland, a. when is aphasia aphasia? the problem of closed head injury. paper presented at the conference of clinical aphasiology, oshkosh, wisconsin, 1982. levin, h.s. aphasia in closed head injury. in m.t. sarno (ed.) acquired aphasia. new york: academic press inc., 1981. levin, h.s., benton, a.l. and grossman, r.g. neurobehavioural consequences of closed head injury. oxford: oxford university press. luria, a.l. traumatic aphasia: its syndromes. psychology and treatment. the hague: mouton, 1970. mcneil, m.r. and prescott, t.e. revised token test. university park press, baltimore, 1978. milton, s.b., prutting, c.a. and binder, g.m. appraisal of communicative competence in head injured adults. paper presented at the clinical aphasiology conference, seabrook island, california, 1984. national institute for personnel research south african wechsler adult intelligence scale. translated and adapted with permission, 1969. , penn, c. a linguistic approach to the detection of minimal language dysfunction in aphasia. unpublished undergraduate research report, department of speech pathdlogy and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, 1972. j penn, c. syntactic and pragmatic aspects of aphasic language. doctoral thesis, department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, 1983. russell, w.r. the traumatic amnesias. london: oxford university press, 1971. sarno, m.t. the nature of verbal impairment after closed head injury. journal of nervous mental disorders, 168, 685692, 1980. sarno, m.t. verbal improvement after chi: report of a replication study. journal of nervous mental disorders, 172, 475479, 1984. / sarno, m.t. and levin, h.s. speech and language disorders after chi. in j.k. darby (ed.) speech and language evaluation in neurology: adult disorders. new york: grune and stratton, 1985. ylvisaker, m. and holland, a.l. in d.f. johns (ed.) clinical management of neurogenic communicative disorders. boston: little brown and co., 1985. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 33, 1986 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ν ε w from amtronix — computerized impedance system z0174 features , ( t t analog meters for compliance and pressure permit easy visualising of test. digital displays for ear canal volume and static compliance. tympanometric pressure may be read digitally. tympanometric gradient is calculated and digitally displayed. digital displays for frequency and intensity for stimuli. expanded reflex mode reveals latency, amplitude, growth and morphology. 5 jolss evoked potential testing with cadweli instrumentation objective detection of audiologic and vestibular dysfunction . the simple solution for complex audiologic cases auditory evoked response to evaluate middleand late-lat'ency responses from the higher centers of the cortex eng to evaluate central and peripheral vestibular dysfunction bsep to objectively localize deficits to the cochlea, the auditory nerve, or the brainstem auditory pathways 40-hertz evoked response audiometry to objectively measure hearing thresholds electroneurography to obtain objective measurements of facial nerve function c audiometry rooms amtronix (pty) ltd., p.o. box 630, bedfordview 2008 phone (011) 615-7647 teletex 4-50033 amtron r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) reviewer acknowledgement open accesshttp://www.sajcd.org.za/ page 1 of 1 the editorial team of the south african journal of communication disorders recognises the value and importance of peer reviewers in the overall publication process – not only in shaping individual manuscripts, but also in shaping the credibility and reputation of our journal. we are committed to the timely publication of all original, innovative contributions submitted for publication. as such, the identification and selection of reviewers who have expertise and interest in the topics appropriate to each manuscript are essential elements in ensuring a timely, productive peer review process. we would like to take this opportunity to thank the following reviewers who participated in shaping this issue of the south african journal of communication disorders: alida ellis amisha kanji anna-mari olivier bea staley christine rogers daleen klop edwin maas fatemah camroodien-surve guilherme m. de carvalho hannelie kemp hilary gardner jaishika seedat jane le roux jenny pahl joanne barratt 75 karyn casey katherine vorster lebogang ramma luanet smit lucia scheepers maggi soer marinda uys meera rijhumal melissa bortz mershen pillay prawin kumar sue rumble ursula l. zsilavecz zandile blose we appreciate the time taken to perform your review successfully. in an effort to facilitate the selection of appropriate peer reviewers for the south african journal of communication disorders, we ask that you take a moment to update your electronic portfolio on www.sajcd.org.za for our files, allowing us better access to your areas of interest and expertise, in order to match reviewers with submitted manuscripts. if you would like to become a reviewer, please visit the journal website and register as a reviewer. to access your details on the website, you will need to follow these steps: 1. log into the online journal at http://www. sajcd.org.za 2. in your ‘user home’ [http://www.sajcd.org. za/index.php/sajcd/user] select ‘edit my profile’ under the heading ‘my account’ and insert all relevant details, bio statement and reviewing interest. 3. it is good practice as a reviewer to update your personal details regularly to ensure contact with you throughout your professional term as reviewer to the south african journal of communication disorders. please do not hesitate to contact me if you require assistance in performing this task. rochelle flint submissions@sajcd.org.za tel: +27 21 975 2602 fax: +27 21 975 4635 south african journal of communication disorders sajcd the autistic child: an approach to handling joyce izikowitz, b.a. log. (rand) johannesburg, south africa 'if a child has a body, but one that will not do what he wants it to—if he has-eyes that see, but do not see things the way other eyes see them—if he has ears that hear, but that have not learned to hear the way other ears do—he cannot tell anyone what his difficulty is, it just seems to him he is always wrong.'16 autism is a syndrome of disturbance, the origin of which is not clear. first described by kanner in 1943, autism was considered as a specific form of childhood psychosis characterized by non-goal directed behaviour, lack of speech and communication, emotional withdrawal and unresponsiveness to the social environment and a fixation with inanimate objects.10 today, two main schools of thought still exist as regards etiology—the organic and the sociological. as a result, manifold theories have been expounded which in turn have provided the blue prints for various approaches in regard to the handling of these children. for many years workers in the field of exceptional children have known therapeutic failure with the non-verbal autistic child, and to date there is little available in the literature in regard to therapeutic handling. the time has come to stop fighting about terminology and labels and to try to understand the problem. the efforts and results of a group of therapists over the past few years are recorded here in the hope that those working in the field of reeducation will find constructive, usable techniques with which to try. to help the 'autistic' child. working principles 1. principle of perceptual orientation. observing the behaviour of the 'autistic' child, we note that he appears to be 'perceptually disorientated'. we think that his 'use' of mannerisms, i.e. rocking, head banging, etc., and his preoccupation with the sameness of objects is ah attempt to investigate and stabilize the environment, as opposed to his reaction to people, who, in an ever-changing situation appear to be a threat to him. it is as if the child cannot organize and attribute meaning to sensory stimuli, and thus behaves in a bizarre manner. 2. principle of child development. a wealth of information has been gathered about normal child development, and we know that maturation is dependent upon physical, mental and emotional growth which proceeds in a well-ordered sequence of events.7·15 journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. , no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the autistic child: an approach to handling 27 piaget" stresses the interaction between the child (genetic endowment) and his environment, and the wholeness of the learning situation. dewey2 stresses the fact that the child's development requires a development of experience. the problem of direction of the desired experience is one of adult selection of appropriate stimuli for instincts and impulses selected for use in the gaining of the new experience. therefore, a structural setting must be piovided with selected stimuli for the child. mowrer13 states that all learning is conditioning, of which problem-solving is a derivative. he stresses the emotional nature of conditioned responses and views behaviour as a continuous on-going function of the informational feedback from all the senses—a function of the total psychological field. 3. principle of brain function. we studied the child within a neurological framework, and applied the techniques one would use for an individual handicapped by: (a) agnosia: all types are characterized by loss of ability to comprehend the meaning, or to recognize the importance of various types of stimulation; these are receptive defects. (nielsen, brain, de jong)> (b) aphasia: a non-functional impairment in the reception, manipulation and/or expression of symbolic content of language. (osgood). (c) apraxia: the inability, as a result of an organic brain lesion, to execute familiar, purposive, more or less automatic movements, when there is neither motor paralysis, sensory disturbance, ataxia or any intellectual impairment. (worster-drought). the sense modalities must function efficiently in order to receive the stimuli, which in turn must be taken via the neuronal pathways to the specific areas for integration and interpretation, before one can respond in the correct way to a situation.1 these principles were used as the foundation for our study project and as the starting point to study each child. perception is a learned process—we learn to see, to hear, to move and to feel in varying stages. the child experiences a situation and reflects back on this experience and so the sequential steps to learning are founded. knowledge of our environment is invaluable to us, as it creates a stability by enabling us to react appropriately. this knowledge is obtained through a spontaneous process of reception of all the sensations, i.e. sensory, motoric, proprioceptive and integrative, of the stimuli in the areas of the brain for concept formation.6·17 (fig. 1.) study project ten children between the ages of fourteen months and five years were seen for initial evaluation. they were tested on all the developmental patterns necessary for the acquisition of learning. a complete history was taken as part of the routine procedure and the following factors (table 1) were listed: age, sex, position in family, siblings, socio-economic backtydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. is, nr. 1: des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) joyce izikowitz the learning process fig. i. ground, birth trauma, milestones of development, speech development, difficulty with eating. symptoms. all the children displayed the following symptoms to some greater or lesser degree: (a) lack of emotion and contact with people; (b) little or no reaction to sound; (c) inadequately developed speech repetoire ranging from mute to a few words used inappropriately; (d) non-goal directed behaviour, i.e. odd mannerisms—whirling around, twisting, flicking movements of hands, head banging, rocking; (e) attachment to objects and perseveration in play; (f) upset at change of routine; (g) non-establishment of laterality and/or cerebral dominance; (h) no body concept or realization that body parts belonged to themselves and that they could control actions; (i) inability to chew or swallow correctly. the ultimate objective was to develop communication and to alter the reinforcing environment so that adequate behaviour was' learned and inadequate behaviour eliminated. therapeutic procedure. the following procedure was used for all the children, with slight variations for the individual child: (a) the stimuli to be presented were carefully selected and presented within a developmental framework; (b) operant conditioning procedures were used: food was used as a reinforcer; the children were fed at the school and taught a hard-food orientation programme. punishment was used for any unacceptable journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. , no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the autistic child: an approach to handling 2 9 behaviour i.e. non-goal directed behaviour such as twirling, head-banging, rocking. effective new behaviour was rewarded and reinforced.9 (c) as it was difficult to determine the stimulus and perceptional repetoire when the child responded to a complex situation, and as we were unable to determine what aspect he was responding to, or what was controlling his response, all the sensory areas were stimulated together in a holistic setting. · · , f (d) verbalization was used continually, following the principles ot luria, i.e. that perception can' be controlled and modified through speech.12 (e) i. aim: the child was made aware of every situation visually, tactually and auditorally e.g. visual attention: the door; tactile: the shape of the door; movement: need to get to, reach out, open and close the door; auditory: the closing sound, the banging sound; speech: continual verbalization was reinforced. . . . . ii. method: this was based on increased stimuli: acquisition and maintaining ot attention was the first step. auditory "1 integrated rewarded visual i > concept > adequate «ngram for tactile j through experience response learning reinforced punishment t incorrect '• response iii. result: a combination of—look hear feel response reinφ 4. φ which led to forced positively see say move or negatively (f) the children were 'blinkered' visually for commands and both rewarded and punished according to behaviour. (g) one therapist worked with the child, together with an aide—they had to adapt to the child and approach him on his level in order to develop a relationship. (h) concrete real-life objects were used—not toys or pictorial representations. (i) conditioned positive reinforcement was immediate. (j) the fixation on objects or visuo-motor perseveration was used constructively and as a starting point for therapy. the child was made actively 'aware'. the holistic approach to therapy ι language development. the prerequisites are that the child must hear sounds and locate and attend to the source. therefore the child was made aware of all sounds, what made the specific noise and from what direction the sound came. speech was moulded on whatever noises the child made which were immediately put into a meaningful context and conditioned positive reinforcement was used. to establish the auditory feedback system, the motor-kinaesthetic technique was used in front of a tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. , nr. 1: des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 30 joyce izikowitz mirror to make the child aware of the various movements made by the oral-peripheral mechanism and to draw his attention to the speaker's face and imitate her movements. the train-ear, speakers and recorders were used to increase the intensity of the sound and to channel the sound directly to the ears. whole words were used at all times.1·5 2. visual development. the child was 'blinkered' and through the aid of torches was taught to fixate visually on any object, follow specific movements, in pursuits and to locate objects. the beam was then discarded and the objects only were presented to*him and he was made to feel each one and follow the placement visually. whatever the child responded to visually was the starting point to illuminate with a torch beam. the original object was then removed and the child would follow only the beam. the beam was then focused on another object. 3. movement development. he was taught how to move all body parts and how to negotiate spatially. the therapist moved his hands and arms through the required movements. he was taught movements along a developmental pattern of crawling, leading through to skilled activities.3·11 4. tactile develop ent the child was made to feel all surfaces and to recognise them through continuous "rubbing". he was taught to reach and grasp objects and move them into required positions and to differentiate between objects through touch alone.3 observations the following interesting factors were noted: 1. speech development. this followed the normal developmental sequence: (a) mouthing of words with or without sound; (b) echolalia, i.e. repetition of what was said to the child, e.g. where ° is the ball?—where is the ball?; (c) repetitions and hesitations, similar to stuttering behaviour; (d) reversal of word order; (e) reversal of pronouns; (f) some difficulty in pronunciation—substitution of his own words; (g) difficulty with abstract thought and logic—transference of reasoning; (h) speech without 'communication'; (i) spontaneous speech in interaction with the environment. 2. behaviour. (a) emotionally they were highly sensitive and anxious. (b) they progressed from passive, still children to hyperactive and destructive, to children with acceptable behaviour. (c) they realized that body parts were attached to themselves and were responsible for specific movements, e.g. full functioning of pencil control, ball playing, etc. ; (d) they showed spontaneous reactions with peers in play. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. , no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the autistic child: an approach to handling 31 d if fi cu lt ie s w it h ea ti ng pr es en t pr es en t pr es en t p re se nt p re se nt sp ee ch d ev el op m en t si le nt ba by d el ay ed s pe ec h t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g m u te si le nt ba by d el ay ed s pe ec h t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g o dd w or ds si le nt ba by d el ay ed s pe ec h t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g o dd w or ds si le nt ba by d el ay ed s pe ec h t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g r an do m n oi se s si le nt ba by d el ay ed s pe ec h t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g m u te m ile st on es o f d ev el op m en tph ys ic al n or m al n or m al n or m al n or m al n or m al b ir th tr au m a in d u ce d ; a no xi a ca es ar ea n se ct io n n o ap pa re nt de fe ct s c ae sa re an se ct io n c or d ar ou nd ne ck so ci oec on om ic b ac kg ro un d m id d le c la ss je w is h m ot h er i n f at he r' s bu si ne ss m id d le c la ss je w is h m o th er — t yp is t f at h er — sa le sm an m id d le c la ss c h ri st ia n m o th er — h ou se w if e f at h er — b us in es sm an l o w i nc om e f at h er — l ab ou re r m o th er — h ou se w if e m id d le c la ss -c at h ol ic e du ca te d si bl in gs 2 b oy s 0 η 1 b o y 1 g ir l 2 si st er s 1 b o y po si ti on in f am ily t h ir d se co n d t h ir d t £ se co nd η co s s s u< s do 14 m on th s 4 ye ar s 5 ye ar s 5 ye ar s 2 ye ar s ca se n o. < w pi ν s m % ph u-t tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. is, nr. : des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 joyce izikowitz d if fi cu lt ie s w it h ea ti ng pr es en t pr es en t pr es en t p re se nt pr es en t sp ee ch d ev el op m en t si le nt b ab y d el ay ed s pe ec h t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g m u te si le nt b ab y d el ay ed sp ee ch t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g m u te si le nt b ab y d el ay ed s pe ec h t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g n oi se s si le nt b ab y d el ay ed sp ee ch t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g n oi se s si le nt b ab y d el ay ed sp ee ch t h o u gh t to b e ha rd o f he ar in g m u te m ile st on es o f d ev el op m en tph ys ic al n or m al i n or m al n or m al n or m al n or m al b ir th tr au m a n o ap pa re nt de fe ct s u nk no w n c ae sa re an se ct io n p re m at ur e r h n eg at iv e t ra n sfu si on s b re ec h pr es en ta ti on so ci oec on om ic b ac kg ro un d m id d le c la ss je w is h f at h er — b us in es sm an m o th er — h ou se w if e m id d le c la ss je w is h f at h er — e d u ca te d m o th er — h ou se w if e l o w i nc om e w or ki ng p ar en ts c hr is ti an lo w so ci oec on om ic c hr is ti an si bl in gs 2 b oy s a do pt ed 2 b oy s a do pt ed o n ly po si ti on in f am ily se co nd fi rs t m id d le fi rs t fi rs t se x t, 2 t, 2 2 a ge 5 ye ar s 2 ye ar s 4 ye ar s 4 ye ar s 2 ye ar s ca se n o. ffl vo 1 q rej ' 00 as < 0 journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. , no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the autistic child: an approach to handling 33 (e) their behaviour was modified through reinforcement and punishment and there was no symptom substitution. (f) they responded spontaneously in situations, and not only to a specific stimulus in a conditioned manner. the children could be handled by more than one person as long as the methods were the same and handling was consistent. it was significant that the four children who progressed the most were the children who were never given the label of 'autism' and therefore were treated as 'normally' as possible, but with increased stimulation. parents of autistic children are as varied in their personalities and the way they deal with children as is any other group of parents. case history child x was 4 years old when we first saw him. he was generally unresponsive to his environment, rocked himself, curled up in the foetal position, masturbated continually, and had not developed speech. he had previously been diagnosed as deaf, aphasic, retarded and autistic. at the age of three years he was enrolled in a nursery school class where he would not communicate, acted in a bizarre manner at times and had to have a constant supervisor. he made no progress and was subsequently referred for speech therapy which was discontinued as x again failed to show any improvement. when he was first assessed by a team of specialists, no firm conclusions were drawn, except that his primary difficulties were consistent with a picture of autism, and that he needed help. salient points from case history. normal pregnancy, but an emergency caesarean section was performed at 7\ months, as there was foetal distress. he was the second child in the family—the sibling being a boy aged 6 years. the parents were educated, middle-class people. physical milestones of development were normal. speech was undeveloped and behaviour bizarre. x was observed daily for a few weeks and a programme was drawn up for, him as follows: (a) he was to be punished for masturbating; (b) as he drooled slightly and could not eat hard foods, but was living on slops, he was to be given meals at the school and taught how to chew and swallow and eat correctly; • (c) his only interest was hub-caps—so all teaching was to be done through hub-caps at first, leading to the reward of a hub-cap for the correct response. this proved very successful, and he was taught visual pursuits, movement size, shape,1 colour, number and reading in this manner. all the previously described techniques were applied. today, x at the age of 6 years, is reading at second grade level in an adjustment group. he has spontaneous speech and spontaneous reaction to people and situations in the environment. he no longer masturbates and only occasionally has a temper tantrum. his vocabulary and speech tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. is, nr. 1: des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 joyce izikowitz are two years above his chronological age, but when upset, he rambles into nonsense jargon, e.g. schoolage for school and garage, and loses logical thought processes. emotionally, he still withdraws, but these periods are few and far between. he plays with other children and reacts spontaneously, laughs, cries and is affectionate to people. he still has a long way to go, and at present we are teaching him logical' thought processes through reading. we have learned much from our successes and our failures, but know far too little and have a great deal to learn. there is no specific treatment for autism, but much can be achieved using the above methods. in reviewing the present situation, we can only arrive at the following conclusions: (a) the necessity for a more holistic approach. (b) the need to treat the cause rather than the symptom. (c) the importance of following the sequence of normal child development within a neurological framework. (d) the need to provide maximum sensory stimulation in all areas in a structured systematic way and to select the stimuli. (e) the need to modify the behaviour through operant conditioning. (f) we are dealing with the problem of labels. it was significant that three of the children who displayed symptoms in keeping with autistic behaviour, when given a maximum sensory stimulation programme were found to be minimally brain injured. summary autism has been considered within a neurological framework of child development. a study has been carried out on ten children. etiology, frequency of symptoms and aspects of therapy have been described. a case study of one child has been presented. opsomming outisme is hier beskou binne die raamwerk van die kind se ontwikkeling. 'n studie is uitgevoer op tien proefpersone (kinders). die etiologie, frekwensie van simptome en aspekte van terapie is beskryf, en 'n frevallestudie van een kind is voorgedra. acknowledgements to the staff of crossroads school—centre of , educational therapy, and forest town school for cerebral palsied children,/' references 1. critchley, m. (1966): the parietal lobe. hafner pub. co., new york, ia. de jong, r. (1967): textbook of ν euro-examination. hoeber. 2. dewey, j. (1902): the child and the curriculum. chicago, university of chicago press. 3. doman-delacato (1968s): proceedings from intensive orientation course— institutes for human porential—philadelphia. 4. ewing, i. r. and ewine, a. w. g. (1961): new opportunities for deaf children. university of london press ltd., warwick square, london e.c.4. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. is, no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the autistic child: an approach to handling 35 5. ε wing, sir a. and ewing, lady e. (1964): teaching deaf children to talk. manchester university press, u.s.a. the volta bureau, washington 7, d.c. 6. foss, β. m. (1966): new horizons in psychology. penguin books ltd., harmondsworth, middlesex, england. 7. gesell, α.: the first five years of life. methuen & co. ltd., london, w.c.2. 8. hebb (1961): organisation of behavior. science editions, inc., new york. 9. hilgard & marquis (1964): (2nd ed.) conditioning and learning. methuen & co. ltd., london. 10. kanner, l. (1949): problems of nosology and psychodynamics of early infantile autism. american journal of orthopsychiatry, 19, 416-26. 11. kephart, n. c. (1965): the slow learner in the classroom. charles e. merrill books, inc. columbus, ohio. 12. luria (1961): the role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behavior. pergamon press, london. 13. mowrer, o. .(1960): learning theory and the symbolic process. 14. piaget, j. (1947): the origin of intelligence in children. international universities press, new york. 15. schilder, p. (1964): contributions to developmental ν euro-psychiatry. international universities press, inc., u.s.a. 16. soffen, η. b. (1965): perceptual training. seminar on children with learning and behavioral problems. borguess hospital, kalamazoo. p. 65-71. 17. vernon, m. d. (1962): the psychology of perception. penguin' books ltd., harmondsworth, middlesex, england. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. , nr. 1: des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) audiometry weber and rinne tests as compared to pure-tone thresholds • thompson, alison κ., b.a. (sp. & η. th.) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) speech therapy department, general hospital, johannesburg summary results of the audiometric weber and rinne tests were compared t o pure t o n e thresholds in 1 8 5 bantu patients. the frequency of 1 0 0 0 hz was selected as being most suitable for weber and rinne testing. t h e weber w a s found t o be of limited diagnostic value even w i t h unilateral c o n d u c t i v e losses whilst the rinne displays a fair degree of efficiency and is of value as a routine supplement t o audiometric threshold tests. opsomming resultate van die o u d i o m e t r i e s e weber en rinne t o e t s e is m e t s u i w e r t o o n drempels vergelyk. 1 8 5 b a n t o e pasiente is as proefpersone gebruik. die frekwensie van looohz is as die geskikste vir die weber en rinne t o e t s e gereken. die weber is, selfs met eensydige geleidings verlies, van beperkte diagnostiese waarde gevind, terwyl die rinne 'n redelike graad van doeltreffendheid g e t o o n het en aanvullend b y die o u d i o m e t r i e s e drempel t o e t s e gebruik kan word. at the hearing clinic at baragwanath hospital, johannesburg, routine audiometric weber and'rinne tests were fairly frequently found to be inconsistent with pure tone thresholds. in the present study, a large number of pure tone audiograms were compared with audiometric rinne and weber results. in recent years, very little has appeared in the literature concerning the weber and rinne tests. that which has appeared deals almost exclusively with testing by means of tuning forks. most authors stress the need to include the weber and rinne in a battery of tuning fork tests including the schwabach and gelle t e s t s . 1 ' 3 , 6 , 8 , 1 1 , 1 ? reliability of these tests has been questioned by these workers but they are nevertheless considered to be useful in supplementing audiometric results. testing by means of the audiometric bone vibrator has certain advantages over testing by means of tuning f o r k s . 2 , 8 , 1 3 the bone vibrator maintains its , intensity output at any desired level, whereas the tuning fork fades rapidly in intensity, especially in the high frequencies. 1 1 in addition, the bone vibrator permits a standard presentation which is independent of the ear of the o p e r a t o r . 5 , 1 3 materials and methods hearing assessments of 185 south african bantu of both sexes with an age range of 12 to 75 years were analysed. these were taken randomly from the tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheeikunde, vol'. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 alison κ. thompson records of patients tested at the hearing clinic over an 18 month period. a maico ma-10 audiometer calibrated to i.s.o. 1964 hearing threshold levels ' was used for testing. the test room was not ideally soundproofed despite the fact that it was lined with acoustic board. conventional pure tone airand bone-conduction audiograms were obtained for each patient. both the weber and rinne were tested at an intensity of 15 db above the patient's threshold of audibility (vibrator placed on the midline of the forehead).8 this is sufficient intensity for clear audibility yet it minimises cross-hearing by being close to the patient's threshold. testing by the weber and rinne methods at several frequencies was considered laborious and unnecessary. a test frequency of 1000 hz was investigated and finally utilised for the following reasons: 1) an increase in the force of vibrator application results in an improved, threshold. the greatest change in intensity due to differential force occurs at 250 hz whilst only slight changes occur at 1000 h z . 4 · 9 2) middle ear lesions influence the vibratory mechanism of the inner ear thereby providing an artefact of poorer bone conduction thresholds.1 although this phenomenon is not yet fully explained, it appears to occur most frequently in the lower frequencies where the conductive loss is generally at a m a x i m u m . 7 3) with lower frequencies the rinne is more likely to be negative in normal ears whereas the reverse is true with higher frequencies. 1 1 the centrally situated 1000 hz is hypothetically more reliable. 4) with frequencies below 1000 hz there is a possibility of confusion between the tactile sensation and hearing. this is enhanced with presentation at higher intensities. 5) a frequency of 1000 hz is situated at the centre of the critical frequency range, and is likely to reflect most successfully both conductive and sensorineural losses. 6) tones below 1000 hz appear to be most affected by ambient noise. this assumes importance when the test environment is not completely soundproof. in rinne testing a conductive loss of 15 db reverses the response at ± 500 hz (i.e. b-c better than a-c), whilst a 20 db loss is required to reverse the response at 1000 h z . 1 0 the test is thus slightly less sensitive at 1000 hz. however, as the levels of odb to 20 db are considered to constitute the normal hearing range, the required sensitivity is present. the audiograms were classified by two audiologists into 11 broad categories of bilateral hearing characteristics. (refer to categories listed in tables i and ii). conventional interpretation criteria were applied. those classified differently were discarded. two assessments were made regarding the weber and rinne results in relation' to each audiogram: 1) correct/error according to the characteristic of airand bone-conduction thresholds at 1000 hz only. journal of lhe south african speech and hearing association, vol. 21, december 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) audiometric weber and rinne tests and pure-tone thresholds 65 2) correct/error as to whether the results reflected the total loss characteristics of the given ear/s as indicated by thresholds over the range of 2508 000 hz'. criteria for assessment of the weber test were based on research by g r o e n . 1 · 3 · 7 lateralisation appears to involve the recognition of interaiiral phase and time as well as intensity differences. lateralisation occurs iri normal ears owing to: a) one stronger vibrating cochlea, as a result of better sound conduction to that cochlea. b) a phase difference between the two sound waves entering the cochleae, the bone conducted tone being lateralised in the cochlea with the leading phase. in normal ears (owing to slight anatomical differences) phase advances may overcome relative amplitude deficiencies of up to 6 db. owing to this phenomenon, a 5 db amplitude difference between bilateral ear thresholds was accepted in the obtaining of a midline response. lateralisation was expected at differences of 10 db or above. criteria for assessment of the rinne test were based on the findings of s h e e h y . 1 0 at 1 000 hz an air-bone gap of less than 20 db is accompanied by a rinne positive response. a rinne negative response occurs with a gap of 20 db or more. theoretically, there is a point around 20 db where both a-c and b-c appear equally loud. this "indifferent" rinne was accepted as correct with an air-bone gap of 20 db only. results and discussion tables i and ii summarise the results in the present study. these were analysed statistically using the cochrans q test, binomial test and poisson test. the results at 1 000 hz only were compared with the total results for each audiogram in order to determine whether the result at 1 000 hz only was able to reflect the total loss characteristics of the ear/s. for both the weber and rinne tests there was no significant difference between the total results and the results at 1 000 hz only (p < 0,05). testing at 1 000 hz is therefore suitable as a test frequency although, clearly, it cannot indicate precipitous high or low frequency loss. the weber test is generally far less efficient than the rinne test. this is demonstrated by the finding that in the weber test, three loss combinations demonstrated highly significant response errors (p < 0,005). these were bilateral normal (a), bilateral equal sensori-neural (e) and bilateral conductive ears (c). these findings are interesting in that combinations a (56,3% error*) and ε (55,5% error) have bilaterally equal cochlear reserve and the weber response should therefore be central. that this does not occur indicates that * all percentages apply to results at 1 0 0 0 hz o n l y . tydskrif van die said-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheeikunde, vol. 21, desember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a lison κ , t hom pson ο e las λ m v . q o o " j t3 ι ο ο ο 73 χ ) ^ [λ ο "ο ^ % » 3 μ ο ο ο ο £ " ω e 3 ο '' ° £ ο οζ. οο ο ο cm ζ . c ο ν τ; ο ο ο e ο 3 ζ ο ό , ο •υ ω ω ο "3 -α ο — ' •s α. = ; ο ω as j2 ω 3 "ε "ε 01 7) ; ν 8 λ 8 .3 η xj _ c £ ο 2 s λ ^ -t1 c prh (l> ο η w ο c5 θ" v νο νθ" γ-1 η co — kh co s ε δ oa ζ ο ο" ν ο ο , θ" v ο ο ζ £ ο ο ο" ν ο cm ο cm ο <υ — > co £ 3 co — c c ο d u oa en" ο n o ο cn) ra s ο ίϋ c/2 ^ ^ c3 ο i— vi 00 -g — 3 • "o o _e ^ ο tn ^ ο 00 ο ο ο ο ϊλ ϊλ « c js c — ο . — co c/3 c2 c/3 journal of the south a frican -speech and h earing a ssociation. v ol. 21. d ecet/tber 1974 reproduced by sabinet gateway under licence granted by the publisher (dated 2012) audiometric weber and rinne tests and pure-tone thresholds 67 pu re t on e t hr es ho ld r es ul ts (b il at er al h ea ri ng c om bi na ti on ) g ui de t o e xp ec te d r es ul ts fo r r in ne % e rr or o f w eb er r es ul ts w he n c om pa re d to t hr es ho ld r es ul ts a t: ο pu re t on e t hr es ho ld r es ul ts (b il at er al h ea ri ng c om bi na ti on ) g ui de t o e xp ec te d r es ul ts fo r r in ne n um be r of pa ti en ts 1 00 0 h z on ly 25 0 h z 8 00 0 h z * (t ot al r es ul ts ) d eg re e of si gn if ic an ce e rr or r es ul ρ a. bilateral both +ve 32 3,1 6,3 no significance normal b. one normal +ve 11,8 11,8 no significance one conductive —ve 34 8,8 17,7 no significance c. bilateral conductive both v e 15 10 10 no significance d. one normal +ve 0 0 no significance one sensorifalse —ve 23 8,7 8,7 no significance neural e. bilateral equal both -i-ve 18 11,1 11,1 < 0 , 0 5 sensori-neural f. bilateral unequal one -l-ve sensori-neural and one false —ve 8 6,3 6,3 no significance g. one normal +ve 0 0 no significance one mixed false —ve 20 30 25 < 0 , 0 5 h. one sensorineural 35,7 35,7 < 0 , 0 5 one mixed 14 21,4 28,6 < 0 , 0 5 1. one sensorineural false — ve 20 20 no significance one conductive v e 5 40 40 no significance j. one conductive —ve 16,7 16,7 no significance one mixed false —ve 6 0 < 16,7 no significance k. bilateral mixed 10 10 10 < 0 , 0 5 * rinne result compared to thresholds over this range to assess agreement with total loss characteristics of ear. table 11. comparison of the agreement between the rinne and pure tone threshold results. tydskrif run die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheeikunde, vol. 21, deseniher 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 alison κ. thompson either the patient resists believing he can hear the tone centrally (in spite of being prepared for this possibility during testing) or that the test is more sensitive than desired in that a slight deficit in the conductive mechanism results in lateralisation in normal ears. no definite trend was found as to the ear selected for response; neither the ear where pathology or loss was judged present nor the better functioning ear was selected. in the bilateral conductive combination (60% error) similar factors are present in that frequently the degree of loss is practically equal in both ears. a central weber was expected, but seldom occurred. although the weber is considered to be primarily a test for determining presence of unilateral conductive loss, results did not support this very well (23,5% error; significant at ρ < 0 , 0 1 ) . the weber appears to be reliable in two combinations only i.e. bilateral unequal sensori-neural hearing loss (25% error) and unilateral sensori-neural hearing loss (13% error). however, in the former case there was only a small group of eight patients and thus these results are questionable. the trend indicates that the weber is more efficient where cochlear reserve is not equal in both ears. an inherent limitation of the weber is that it is not possible to interpret responses obtained when a mixed loss is present, or when there is a combination of sensori-neural loss in one ear and conductive loss in the other. the response obtained, from the patient may thus prove misleading. on considering the results for the rinne, all results for similar categories i.e. normal, conductive, etc, were compared to assess whether results for any given category were comparable irrespective of the hearing characteristics of the contralateral ear. on analysis, results within each category did not differ significantly (all at ρ < 0,05) thus confirming test consistency. the rinne is generally efficient in depicting normal hearing and conductive losses. (an arbitrary response was usually given with a conductive loss around 20 db, thus reducing efficiency in this region). ears with a sensori-neural loss demonstrated a significant incidence of errors (p < 0,05) in only two combinations i.e. with a mixed loss (35,7%) and in the bilateral equal sensorineural group (11,1%). the latter is of borderline significance only. sensorineural loss is therefore depicted less efficiently but results are still of value in diagnosis. mixed losses give the least reliable results. the use of masking to prevent the false-negative response in unilateral severe sensori-neural loss has long been a problem. the general difficulties in establishing effective masking levels are equally relevant here. the writer suggests that the false-negative response be accepted as it stands. in the present study, masking was not used. however, on analysis of results, the false-negative appears consistently where one would expect it to occur. the history, plus weber and schwabach tests, can be employed to provide further information (the present study found the weber to be reliable in this category). there is, nevertheless, no totally satisfactory solution to ;this problem. the greatest limitation of the weber and rinne tests is that inconsistencies may occur in any patient's responses for a variety of psycho-acoustic, ana. journal of llic jioulh african spccch and hc iy74 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) audiometric weber and rinne tests and pure-tone thresholds 69 tomical and physical reasons. as with any test requiring a subjective response, these tests are only as efficient as the patients' preparedness and ability to judge what is presented to him. unsophisticated patients may be more suggestible and erratic in this regard. although findings based on the audiometric weber and rinne cannot be directly compared to those obtained with tuning forks, owing to the slight differences in frequency levels and presentation, trends of test efficiency can be extracted. neither of these tests are sufficiently reliable to substitute for threshold tests. however, the findings of this study suggest that the rinne is of value as a supplement to threshold testing. a result at variance with the threshold, levels alerts the tester to the need for closer investigation. the weber can be used reliably to help identify a unilateral sensori-neural loss but, clearly, its general inefficiency makes it of little diagnostic value. acknowledgements i wish to thank dr b.l. wolfowitz for his suggestions on presentation and mrs j.e. anderson for assisting with classification of results. also, mr g.l. kimble and the department of statistics, c.h.d., johannesburg for the statistical analysis; and dr l. faivelsohn, medical superintendent of baragwanath hospital for his permission to publish. references 1. allen, g.w. and fernandez, c. (1960): the mechanism of bone conduction. annals of otol, rhinol, and laryngol, 69, 5-28. 2. davis, h. and silverman, s. (1970): hearing and deafness, 3rd edition holt, rinehart & winston, new york. 3. groen, j.j. (1962): the value of the weber test. in schuknecht, h.f. (ed.), otosclerosis international symposium, chap. 14. little, brown & co., massachusetts. 4. harris, j.d., haines, h.l. and myers, c.k.,(1953): a helmet-held bone conduction vibrator. laryngoscope, 63, 998-1007. 5. jesberg, s. (1923): recording of functional hearing tests. laryngoscope, 33, 379-383. 6. johnson, e.w. (1970): tuning forks to audiometers and back again. laryngoscope, 80, 49-68. 7. naunton, r.f. (1963): the measurement of hearing by bone conduction. in jerger, j . ( e d . ) , modern developments in audiology, chap. 1. academic press, new york. 8. newby, h.a. (1965): audiology principles and practice, 2nd edition. vision press, london. 9. nilo, e.r. (1968): the relation of vibrator surface area and static application force to the vibrator-to-head coupling. j. of speech and hearing res., 11,805-810. ivdskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak-en (jehoorheelkunde, vol. 21. detember 1974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 alison κ. thompson 10. sheehy, j.l. et al. (1971): tuning fork tests in modern otology. arch otolarvng., 94, 132-138. 1 1. sonnenschein, r. (1933): fundamental principles of functional hearing tests. arch. otolaryng., 18,599-613. 12. tschiassny, k. (1946): tuning fork tests. annals of otol., rhinol., and laryngol., 55,423-430. 13. watson, l.a. and tolan, t. (1949): hearing tests and hearing instruments. williams and wilkins, baltimore. ja u d i o m e t r y equipment hearing aids noise control and and maico iriteracoustics grason-stadler siemens mad sen widex willco eckstein linco h i l l a r y r e i c h e n b e r g e r i c c. l e w i s / the needler westdene hearing aid organisation (pty) ltd. p.o. box 28975, sandr i n g h a m 2131 t e l e p h o n e : 45-7262 journal of the soulli african speech anil hearing associaiion. i nl j/, december 19~4 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) do you have an illegal racket? government notice r2237 of 3 0 / 1 1 / 7 3 and the onus is on you to ensure their prosets d o w n m a x i m u m safe noise levels in factories and workshops. it has been done to protect your workers from damage to their hearing. lection. a sure w a y of c o m p l y i n g w i t h the law is to u s e b r u e l & k j a e r sound level meters. they conform to natonal and international standards. β &· κ personal noise dose m e t e r : it's c o m p a c t , p o c k e t sized a n d p r a c t i c a l . t h e w o r k e r w e a r s it w h e r e v e r he w o r k s w h e r e v e r he g o e s . h i s d a i l y n o i s e d o s e is m e a s u r e d . c o n f o r m s t o i e c ft 1 2 3 . β & κ miniature sound level meters: e s s e n t i a l t o o l s for n o i s e a b a t e m e n t e n g i n e e r s , i n d u s t r i a l h y g i e n i s t s a n d all o t h e r s w h o s e j o b it is t o k e e p n o i s e a t t o l e r a b l e l e v e l s . m e a s u r e s t o i e c s t a n d a r d s for s o u n d l e v e l m e t e r s . sole distributors: t e l k o r / / / / / j o h a n n e s b u r g : 29 w e b b e r sireet, setby, p . 0 box 776 4 . phone 8 3 6 1 3 0 1 . c a p e t o w n : 41 2 voortrekker road. p.o. box 26. m a i l l a n d c p. phone 51 3311. p o r t e l i z a b e t h : 4 0 u i t e n h a g e road. p.o. box 2 0 0 1 , p.e. phone 2 4 0 8 1 . d u r b a n : p.o. box 729. durban. associated w i t h dowson & dobson ltd. l'lienck & a s s o o a journal o the south african spe and hearing association, vol. 21, december 974 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) amplivox hearing services (pty.) ltd. suite 322-3 bosman building, cor. eloff & bree streets p.o. box 9076 johannesburg tel. 23-6419 23-6431 we are suppliers of the following: hearing aids amplivox,dahlberg,oticon, rextonand cosmocord — covering all types of hearing aids. , audiometers amplivox, amplaid, peters and tracor manual and automatic audiometers of all types. i acoustic booths and sound level meters tracor, amplivox and acos. deaf school equipment peters, connevans and amplivox speech training and group hearing aids. ear defenders gunfender, sonex, super sonex, interceptor, auralgard, sonogard and supamuff from amplivox. / / amplivox hearing services t h e a c k n o w l e d g e d experts r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a i d s f o r • the development of perception • the acquisition of speech and language skills • the improvement of motor co-ordination • helpful texts for therapists • educational toys, books and equipment • records for auditory training • catalogues on request • large variety of tests recently arrived ° play and schoolroom m (adjoining the constantia cinema) t e l e p h o n e s : 42-5350; 42-6529. p.o. box 52137, saxonwold, tvl. p l u s c o n s u l t 8 t y r w h i t t avenue, rosebank r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society objective psychotherapy in the treatment of dysphemia arnold a. lazarus clinical psychologist (regd.) since many speech therapists feel that dysphemia is already "the disorder of too many theories," they regard outside opinion as an intrusion and a usurpation of their rights. others, imbued with a need for therapeutic teamwork, (a high-sounding concept which rarely works in practice) often err by accepting contradictory and fragmentary views which only,further confuse and complicate the issue. the present paper is an endeavour to increase the scope and range of the speech therapist's role by offering an objective rationale for the inclusion of certain behaviour therapeutic* techniques into the sphere of logopedics. the study of dysphemia may be conveniently divided into three parts: (i) the actual stutter (i.e. the analysis of clonic and tonic spasms associated with phonation, articulation and respiration which disturb the flow of speech. this would include accompanying tics and allied patterns of dysrhythmia in various areas of psychomotor activity).. (ii) the onset and underlying causes of stuttering. (in this connection, it should be emphasized at the very outset that attempts to reveal the genesis of stuttering through biochemical analyses, psychometric investigations, medical and neurological examinations, e e g recordings and the like have all proved nonspecific for any stutterlike pattern). (iii) the stutterer's psychological responses, with special reference to his attitudes and feelings in various speaking situations. in dealing with the problems associated with (iii), the speech therapist is handicapped by a paucity of effective techniques. on occasion, the anxieties which often exacerbate dysphemic responses are glossed over. in general reassurance or mild emotional support is offered, while relaxation and specific "assignments" are used as therapeutic adjuncts. on the other hand, many problems are left alone on the assumption that it is highly dangerous to *we are following eysenck's example of subsuming the theoretical concepts and practical methods of treatment derived from modern learning theory under the heading "behaviour therapy." dabble in psychotherapy. this is a crucial gap. in most practical essentials the speech therapist is willy-nilly a psychotherapist and the treatment of specific neuroses which have a bearing on the mechanisms of speech, may legitimately be placed within the province of logopedics. the present article outlines two techniques which, in time, may conceivably form an integral part of the therapeutic "modus operandi" of every speech therapist. although it has not been established that dysphemia is essentally a manifestation of unresolved conflicts and anxiety, even the pure organicists cannot deny that a stutterer's speech pattern usually deteriorates in anxietygenerating situations. it is empirically demonstrable that attitudes of hypersensitivity and self-consciousness tend to further inhibit the stutterer's verbalization and result in "secondary blocking." some stutterers, burdened by pervasive anxieties, find the mere thought of speech terrifying. the desensitization technique outlined below is not for them; it is indicated in cases where the individual is overwhelmed by anxiety and tension in specific speaking situations. it must be understood, however, that neither of the therapeutic techniques* dealt with is intended as a "cure" for dysphemia. when working towards a cure, the emphasis should be on a synthesis of different therapeutic procedures, so that consideration is given to the entire speech mechanism perse and to the socio-psychobiological features. but at the present stage of our knowledge, the complete elimination of a confirmed stutter is generally a therapeutic ideal rather than a practical objective. therapeutic idealism often results in objective nihilism and fails to achieve even those modest therapeutic goals which are well within the limits of our practical skills. those theorists (such as the pschoanalysts) for example, who insist on treating the so-called "total personality" are often so absorbed in the intricacies of their amorphous task, that they rarely achieve results comparable with *the practical application of learning theory to the treatment of tics which often accompany stutterlike patterns is dealt with towards the end of this paper. speech therapists will easily recognize the different emphasis which is placed on the well-known technique of "negative practice." r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) march j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society those attained by therapists who use only simple vocal exercises. thus, on the assumption that it is wise to proceed with scientific humility and caution, we shall now outline two techniques which aim to alleviate rather than to eliminate the problem of dysphemia. (i) s y s t e m a t i c d e s e n s i t i z a t i o n b a s e d o n r e l a x a t i o n 4 w o l p e 2 has shown that specific anxieties can be eliminated if they are progressively opposed by muscular relaxation. thus, if a stutterer becomes anxious each time he answers the telephone, this response (anxiety) must be opposed by a new response (e.g. relaxation) which is physiologically incompatible with anxiety. the bond between the specific speaking situation and the anxiety will then be broken. this fact was clearly demonstrated in the case of a 19-year-old pharmacy student whose mild stutter became extremely pronounced each time he had to answer the telephone. " a s soon as the 'phone starts ringing i begin to feel butterflies in my stomach," he explained. " a s i get near the 'phone my fears get worse and by the time i lift the receiver to my ear, i just know that i'm going to stutter . . . by then i can't even open my mouth." he added that the mere thought of speaking on a telephone made him feel ansxious. systematic desensitization was applied as follows:he was first trained in an accelerated version of jacobson's3 progressive relaxation. while fully relaxed, he was asked to imagine the sound of a telephone ringing in the distance. (he was told to signal to the therapist if he experienced any feelings of anxiety while visualizing any of the given situations). as this failed to provoke any anxiety, he was asked to imagine the sound of a telephone ringing in the same room. this image also failed to generate any anxiety, but the thought of a telephone ringing right next to him provoked a fair measure of anxiety. his anxiety was opposed by relaxation again and again until he was able to tolerate, with complete tranquillity, the idea of a telephone ringing right beside him. the patient was seen three days later. he reported that he no longer experienced any anxiety when he actually heard the telephone ringing . . . "the butterflies are completely gone in that situation." he was then desensitized to the thought of approaching a ringing telephone. it required four sessions before he was able to *for a complete practical and theoretical exposition of systematic desensitization based on relaxation see wolpe2 chapter 9. contemplate picking up the receiver with no feelings of anxiety. at this stage he reported that his phobia for telephones had greatly diminished. "i don't panic any longer," he stated, "but i still stutter very badly over the 'phone . . . it's worst of all when i try checking an order over the 'phone." after nine additional desensitization sessions, there was no apparent difference between his telephonic speech and his verbalization in face-to-face situations. at the time of writing, he has maintained his improvement for over four years. equally good results were achieved in the case of a 19-year-old student whose stutter incapacitated her while out on a "date," while speaking in class and when answering the telephone. these three anxiety areas were treated concurrently and required 22 sessions for their complete elimination. the patient also reported an improvement in many general aspects of her speech. the follow-up in this case is also over four years. similarly, a 42-year-old business executive who had experienced great difficulty when talking to important clients and when ordering in a restaurant stated that "my new business contacts don't believe me when i tell them that i am a stutterer." he required only 13 desensitization sessions to effect this improvement. a case reported elsewhere4 was that of a 34-year-old engineer who received desensitization therapy for a speech disturbance characterized by lengthy and frequent "word blocks" accompanied by considerable tension and facial grimaces. when first interviewed he stuttered on about 12-25% of words, with "blocks" averaging 3-4 seconds. his attitude towards speaking situations was poor. he received 30 hours of therapy over 9 months. therapy sessions were usually held once a week. training in progressive relaxation was followed by systematic desensitization. among others the following anxiety-situations were treated: time pressures (especially speaking on the telephone as he conducted many of his occupational affairs by long-distance calls), telling jokes, public speaking, difficult 'audiences' i.e. specific people who provoked added speech difficulties. progress was gradual, but at the termination of therapy a substantial gain in speech fluency had been achieved. one of the principal skills in the administration of systematic desensitization is to proceed at a pace which is in keeping with the patient's level of anxiety. n o harm seems to ensue from proceeding at a pace that might prove too slow for a patient, but too rapid a pace can prove extremely antitherapeutic and lead to increased levels of anxiety. the desensitization procedure r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society can be used with children5 but, as yet no one seems to have administered it to dysphemic children. (ii) t h e u s e o f m a s s e d p r a c t i c e in t h e t r e a t m e n t o f t i c s a s s o c i a t e d w i t h d y s p h e m i a yates 0 deduced a method of eliminating neurotic tics by building up a habit of "not performing the tic." according to hullian theory7 8 9 massed practice of a motor activity (e.g. a tic) causes reactive inhibition (ir) to build up. when ir reaches a certain critical point the subject requires rest i.e. he experiences a need not to perform the tic. t h e habit of not performing the tic becomes associated with drive reduction and is therefore reinforced. repeated massed practice will therefore build up a negative habit ("not-doing-the-tic") which will militate against the positive habit of doing the tic. yates's theoretical model was applied in the case of an 18-year-old youth with an extreme stutter who invariably twisted his mouth, screwed up his eyes and jerked his head forward and back during a "block." twelve years of intermittent speech therapy had been of no avail. he was referred to the writer for vocational guidance and was advised first to undergo therapy for the pronounced spasms and tics which seemed to impede his speech. t h e tics were treated concurrently but independently. each tic was given five one-minute periods of massed practice, with one minute's rest between each period. the same order of massed practice was employed throughout the treatment. he was first required to practice the jerking of his head for five trials. after three minutes rest he was asked to perform the mouth twisting movements and finally reproduced the eye-movements. the patient was instructed to carry out two sessions daily. he was supervised by the therapist twice a week. the tendency to screw up his eyes during a "block" was eliminated in less than three weeks. the mouth-twisting response and the head-jerking required more than a month of massed practice before they entirely disappeared. t o date, there has been no apparent symptom substitution, nor have any of the original tics or spasms returned. the overall improvement in his speech is really quite remarkable. his blocks are now far more infrequent and they are usually so momentary that they often pass completely unoticed by untrained observers. a prolonged follow-up of this case is being undertaken. d i s c u s s i o n it is premature at this stage, of course, to assess the value of the techniques outlined above in the treatment of dysphemia. the preliminary findings, however, are most encouraging and warrant further investigation. this introduces the query: " w h o should carry out the treatment, speech therapist, psychologist, or both?" w e therefore return to the consideration of therapeutic teamwork. in the opinion of the writer, therapeutic teamwork is tenable only where there is a clear-cut division of the skills involved. in the case of a therapeutic liaison between doctor and psychologist, for instance, the collaboration is usually fruitful. this is because the doctor remains responsible for the physical health of the patient and the therapeutic lines of demarcation are reasonably obvious to patients and therapists alike. it is difficult to decide whether therapeutic teamwork between speech therapist and psychologist is advisable — so much depends on their respective theoretical orientations, their therapeutic objectives, the patient's level of adjustment and so forth. by and large, it is our view that the speech therapist, given the necessary training," would be adequately qualified to " g o it alone" when confronted with cases similar to those presented above. *it must be understood that the desensitization technique is a highly specialised procedure. the therapist who employs desensitization requires tuition in (a) the construction of the relevant anxiety hierarchies (b) the application of hypnotic and ordinary relaxation procedures (c) the handling of anxiety which is aroused during a session (d) in assessing the optimal number and duration of the stimuli which should be presented in any given session. r e f e r e n c e s 1. e y s e n c k , h. j . (1959) " l e a r n i n g t h e o r y and b e h a v i o u r t h e r a p y . " j . ment. sc., 105:61. 2. wolpe, j . (1958) p s y c h o t h e r a p y b y reciprocal i n h i b i tion. s t a n f o r d university p r e s s and w i t w a t e r s r a n d u n i v e r s i t y p r e s s . 3. j a c o b s o n , e . (1938) p r o g r e s s i v e r e l a x a t i o n . c h i c a g o : u n i v e r s i t y of chicago p r e s s . 4. l a z a r u s , a. a. and r a c h m a n , s. (1957). " t h e u s e of s y s t e m a t i c d e s e n s i t i z a t i o n in p s y c h o t h e r a p y " . s . afr. med. 3. 31:934. 5. l a z a r u s , a.a. (1959) " t h e e l i m i n a t i o n of c h i l d r e n ' s p h o b i a s b y d e c o n d i t i o n i n g . " med. proc., 5:261. 6. y a t e s , a. (1958) " t h e application of l e a r n i n g t h e o r y to the t r e a t m e n t of t i c s . " j . abnorra. soc. p s y c h o l . 56:175. 7. hull, c. l . (1943) p r i n c i p l e s of b e h a v i o u r . n e w york appleton, c e n t u r y crofts. 8. hull, c. l . (1951) e s s e n t i a l s of behaviour. n e w h a v e n : yale u n i v e r s i t y p r e s s . 9. hull, c. l. (1952) a b e h a v i o u r s y s t e m . new h a v e n : yale u n i v e r s i t y p r e s s . r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) editorial in march 1962, professor c. j. dreyer succeeded professor hurst as chairman of the executive committee. we welcome him most cordially and assure him of our cooperation at all times. this issue of the journal of the south african logopedic society consists of articles by overseas contributors. ruth clark worked in south africa some years ago and we are privileged to publish her review of recent research on the parietal lobes, proprioceptive sensibilities and tactilc and kinesthetic abilities in speech production — avenues of investigation which offer exciting possibilities for future work. felix trojan and herta weihs have provided us with some idea of the continental approach to speech and voice therapy. their discussion of factors relevent to a correct diagnosis of a speech or voice disorder covers a wide field and is most interesting. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationships between measurements of stuttering behaviour myrtle l . aron, p h . d . department of logopedics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg in our attempt to ascertain the symptomatology of stuttering and the change and progress that might occur in particular situations and in therapy, evaluative procedures become essential. measurements of the dimensions of stuttering behaviour is part of the evaluative procedure. not all the dimensions have been established—there are many facets which remain elusive in terms of objective assessment. stuttering varies a great deal under certain known conditions but it can also vary for no apparent reason. the cyclic phenomenon in stuttering has been discussed by quarrington,11 and others, but it has not been explained satisfactorily.. an objective study of variability in stuttering is therefore difficult to make as any change that may occur may not necessarily be associated with manipulated conditions or the process of therapy. individual personality differences add to the problem of objective measurement in stuttering. however, these factors need not preclude our use of measurements and some attempt must be made to quantify data. this is not to say that we must belittle qualitative judgements. these finally are the most important. in many respects those measurements we can make at the present time reflect qualitative assessments. the need to attempt measurement, although we may not have all the answers, is supported by thurstone18 who states that: . . . it is better to formulate the law of comparative judgements in terms of the discriminal error, which is a psychological concept, than to wait until we shall understand physiologically . . . while conducting a study to ascertain the effects of a combination of tranquillizing and sedatory agents on the symptoms of stuttering, an array of measurements were used to assess stuttering speech. it is not the intention here to report on the effects of the drug as this has been considered elsewhere,1· 2 but it is relevant to report on the type of measurements made of the stuttering symptom and their relationships to each other. this latter aspect can be considered independently of any drug effects on the subjects who participated in the study. criterion of what constitutes stuttering. t h e construct and application of measurements and ratings of stuttering must take into account a criterion of what constitutes stuttering. this becomes particularly important when differentiations must be made between the non-fluencies that can be detected in normal speakers and stutterers. johnson has observed that "interjections, revisions, and phrase repetitions" can sometimes be considered as "normal" disfluencies.7 boehmler had indicated that two groups of judges (trained and untrained) classified part-word repetitions as "stuttering" as journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) yrtle l . aron frequently for stutterers as for non-stutterers.4 the judges agreed more when stuttering was severe and when "normal" non-fluencies were mild. williams and kent found that syllable repetitions and prolongations were more consistently identified as "stuttering" by judges.21 johnson listed eight features of speech which he thought to be representative of disfluencies and this became known as the iowa speech disfluency test.7 young used a modified version of this test, and considered five rather than the eight features, in view of the rarity of occurrence of "phrase repetitions, incomplete phrases and broken words."23 his categories were: (a) interjections. (b) part-word repetitions. (c) word-phrase repetitions (johnson considered these as two separate features). (d) prolongations, including broken words (johnson considered these as two separate features). (e) revisions, including incomplete phrases (johnson considered these as two separate features). young then used multiple correlation procedures to analyse the speech samples of stutterers as rated by listeners.23 as a result of these procedures he modified his categories further and this was the criterion adopted in the study under discussion. the categories were: (a) syllable and sound repetitions. (b) sound prolongations. (c) broken words (or broken utterances). (d) words involving apparent unusual stress or tension. in addition to the above, the writer added word repetitions with "syllable and sound repetitions" as she feels that this feature occurs fairly frequently, especially on short words. the criterion followed appears feasible to use as a classification for denoting stuttering moments and rating the severity of stuttering. severity ratings of stuttering. various measurements can be applied to ascertain the severity of stuttering. physiological changes can be noted such as alterations in· breathing, heart rate, electrical skin response, etc. the duration of the stuttering moments can also be measured. in a personal communication, van riper pointed out that there is still no adequate way of measuring the severity of stuttering.19 he felt that the problem lies in the fact that stuttering is a compound of fear and struggle. he considered the use of physiological measurements but suggested at the same time that these were subject to too many variables and would differ from subject to subject according to the nature of their symptoms. the use of physiological measurements, although theoretically plausible, were, for practical reasons, rejected for the purposes of this study. one satisfactory measurement of the severity of stuttering which has received some attention over recent years, is the use of an "equal-appearing intervals scale" to rate severity based on audible characteristics. the use of such a scale represents a classic psychophysical method. the subject (or experimenter) sorts a number of stimuli into specified categories where the intervals appear to the experimenter to be equal. sherman has done a great deal of work on severity scales, and together with lewis, she first applied a nine-point scale consisting of equal-appearing intervals where ι represented "least severe stuttering" and 9 represented journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationships between measurements of stuttering behaviour 17 "most severe stuttering."15 fifteen judges rated speech samples on this scale and their judgements were found to be satisfactorily reliable. there was evidence that this severity scale had internal consistency. this scale was applied to permanent records (tape recordings) of the audible characteristics of stuttering. sherman then used the same scale to ascertain if observers could make reliable judgements of continuous stuttered speech.13 she demonstrated that this method is reliable, and is experimentally and clinically useful for assigning a rank order position of severity. johnson suggests a seven-point equal-appearing intervals scale where he presents a description of the type of stuttering that each rating represents.8 cullinan et al. compared five-, seven-, .and nine-point equal-appearing intervals scales and found that the scales were significantly correlated and that reliable values could be obtained from any one of these.5 apart from measures of the severity of stuttering, the frequency with which stuttering occurs must contribute to the listeners' evaluation of the severity of the symptom. this aspect was studied by sherman and trotter who questioned the relationship between the frequency count of the moments of stuttering and measures of severity made on an equal-appearing intervals scale.16 from the data presented by eleven observers, they found that these two aspects were significantly and positively correlated, but they do point out that the relationship between the two measures is not strong enough to allow one measure tô predict the other. sherman et al. compared three measures of stuttering severity: (a) reading time. (b) frequency moments of stuttering. (c) scale values from listeners' ratings.17 all interrelationships were found to be statistically significant and the strength of the relationship was highest between the frequency counts and the rated severity. the visual characteristics of stuttering may be as important to measure as the audible characteristics when making severity ratings. it is difficult, however, to obtain such information for permanent records due to the expense of filming. it is necessary to establish, nevertheless, how close or how far severity ratings based on audible characteristics differ from those based on visual and audible features. in this regard a detailed study was conducted by williams et al., who studied the ratings of stuttering by audio, visual and audiovisual cues making use of synchronized photography and sound recording.22 they found that the frequency count of stuttering and scale values representing severity are more reliable when obtained by audio and audiovisual observation than when obtained by visual observation. they suggested that the use of audio cues alone is sufficient for obtaining useful and reliable measures of the frequency and severity of stuttering. it is possible, the present writer feels, to consider the relative importance and influence of visual cues when marking the severity ratings of stuttering during the stutterers' act of reading or speaking, where audible cues would also be· present apart from the visual characteristics. if the speech is recorded and played back at a later stage when the memory of the visual cues ceases to be influential, comparative data of tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) yrtle l . aron ratings could indicate whether visual cues added significantly to the initial ratings of severity. johnson has said that the advantage of a severity rating scale with stutterers ensures a certain degree of uniformity and comparability of judgements.8 he cautions that such a rating can have its limitations and that clinical judgements of stutterers are still important. a relevant point that can be considered here is the reliability of one observer to make severity ratings of stuttering. cullinan et al. found that judges tend to agree better with themselves than with other judges.5 their evidence suggests that it is not sufficiently reliable to use a single judge on a single rating scale. it would seem feasible and convenient to employ a single observer to make severity ratings which can be compared with ratings made by a group of judges. this should indicate the kind of agreement among the judges and the reliability of the single observer's judgements. sherman studied this aspect by comparing individual observers and found, in contrast to cullinan et al., that reliable scale values can be obtained from a single observer.14 frequency of stuttering moments. the frequency with which stuttering occurs contributes to the severity of the stuttering symptom. like severity, frequency of stuttering can increase or decrease, depending on the tension and anxiety felt by the stutterer in communicative situations. the counting of stuttering moments constitutes a basic measurement of the disorder. it is customary to have a subject read a passage and to mark the words stuttered on. it is also possible to count the frequency of stuttering during spontaneous speech where tape recordings can be transcribed and the moments marked and studied. as with the severity ratings of stuttering, visual cues exhibited by the stutterer can influence the experimenter's markings of the moments of stuttering while the subject is present and reading. two important dimensions of stuttering behaviour are calculated on the basis of the frequency count—adaptation and the consistency effect. adaptation. this is a well-known phenomenon in stuttering where stuttering moments tend to decrease progressively with successive oral readings of the same passage. there appear to be individual differences, however, where some stutterers do not show any adaptation or may even produce increased stuttering with every reading.8 newman considered the possibility of classifying stutterers into groups on the basis of their adaptation performances.10 he re-examined data on adaptation and found that there were instances where subjects did not reveal any adaptation. the first study to note the phenomenon of adaptation in stuttering was conducted by van riper and hull in 1934.20 they attributed the decrease in the stuttering moments to the stutterer's subjective adaptation, either to the situation or to the reading material. since then many studies have been published on this aspect and the general attitude appears to be that the stutterers experience less anxiety with each reading, thereby stuttering less. consistency. consistency effect of stuttering was first noted by johnson and knott who found a significant -tendency for subjects to stutter on the same words from reading to reading of the same.passage.9 johnson et al. feel that the importance of knowing the consistency in stuttering is that it may indicate journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationships between measurements of stuttering behaviour 19 how strongly stuttering responses are associated with stimuli or cues that have been conditioned.8 the anticipation and expectancy of stuttering is closely related to the consistency effect. reading rate. it has been commonly observed that stutterers take longer to read than non-stutterers. this time factor contributes to the severity of the problem. sander reported that reading rate was a highly stable measure of stuttering severity.12 he considered the relationship between reading rate and the frequency moments of stuttering, and concluded that the two measurements can be used as a tool for evaluating the speech improvement of stutterers. the computation of the reading rate in words per minute is outlined by johnson et a!.s the number of words contained in a reading passage is divided by the number of seconds taken to read it and this is multiplied by sixty to convert the final rate to words per minute. this measurement, requiring only the use of a stop-watch, is perhaps one of the most objective methods of assessing any change in stuttering. the severity of the stuttering symptom, together with the frequency of its occurrence, will directly influence the rate of reading. procedure in present study the study extended over fifteen weeks which were divided into five periods of three weeks each. forty-six subjects participated—37 were europeans (mean age: 26 years 6 months) and 9 were africans (mean age: 20 years 1 month). a number of testing procedures was administered to subjects individually once in three weeks. the purpose of these three-weekly assessment-interviews was to collect data pertaining to various aspects of the subjects' stuttering. apart from the measurements of aspects of stuttering, the subjects also completed a daily questionnaire for the duration of the experiment and made verbatim reports, but results of these are not pertinent to the subject matter under consideration. although the measurement of aspects of stuttering was done with a view to ascertaining the efficacy of medication, the measurements can be studied in isolation without any spurious effect from the drug. the purpose of this present report is to indicate the interand intra-relationships between the measurements themselves. m a t e r i a l the reading passage utilized was a slight modification of a portion of the "rainbow" passage by fairbanks.6 the passage was modified so that it could be divided into two sections—the first part consisting of 200 words, and the second part of n o words. during the first three assessment periods only the 200 word passage was presented, and during the last two assessment periods the complete 310 word passage was given. the extra n o word passage was included in order to ascertain whether any adaptation to the same passage (first 200 words) had occurred. thus the n o word passage, as the nonadaptation passage, acted as a control. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 .. · myrtle l·.' aron : i. experimenter's severity' rating of stuttering on subjects'. reading. the rating .of the severity of stuttering was made by the experimenter. after the subject had read the passage. .. ' (a) the rating was made on a nine-point equal-appearing intervals· scale where " i " represented no. stuttering, "9v very, severe stuttering, and· "5" represented average stuttering. the other points on this scale represented values falling between these points. the.rating was made by'the experimenter immediately after, the subject hadcompleted his reading. · s ; · • ' *. · (b) a second rating on the same scale as described above was made when the tape-' recording of the reading was played back. this was done soon after the subject left the experimenter and' it \was felt necessary since it was possible that visual cues displayed by the subjects during the' reading might have affected the first severity rating made: . * v' . . (c) a.third severity rating was made, without reference'to the above two ratings, using the same scale. this was made when the tape-recording was played back approximately 10 days after the initial reading. this third rating was based predominantly on auditory characteristics. it was also considered a crucial one in that samples of taperecordings were to be played" to judges who would react only to these auditory cues, thus making comparative data possible. ..· 2. experimenter's severity rating of stuttering on subjects' spontaneous conversation. this rating was made on the same nine-point equalappearing intervals scale described above. spontaneous conversation took place when the subjects first arrived for the interview. at .the end of the interview, if the information was not volunteered in enough detail, the subjects were asked to comment on their speech, on the study, etc. this rating was' niade separately from the readings as it was felt that with many stutterers,. there is a marked disparity between the amount of stuttering in reading and in spontaneous speech performances. no attempt was made to structure the conversation and no tape recording was made. > 3. subjects'. severity rating on their own stuttering in reading. after a subject had read the passage he was asked to rate himself on the same scale used in the above measurements. the instruction to the subject was as follows: on the following scale, rate your stuttering on the passage you have just read. 1 2 . 3 4 5 6 7 8 ' 9 n o average very severe stuttering stuttering stuttering 4. subjects' severity rating of their own stuttering on a playback of the recording of the reading. approximately thirty minutes after the subject had recorded the reading passage, he was asked to hear the tape recording and to rate himself on the same scale. the instruction to the subject was: . the reading you have just made will be played back to you. listen to it and rate your stuttering on the following scale:' / the scale was presented in the same manner as in 3 above. it was felt that · the confrontation of hearing their speech might change their judgement of the severity which they had previously made on their reading. it was expected that, due to poor attitudes towards speech, the subjects would make a slightly higher rating than previously. this information could be of value for comparative purposes, in terms of self-judgements of the severity of stuttering based on the same material judged under different conditions. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1:. september 196j. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationships between measurements of stuttering behaviour 21 5. frequency count of stuttering moments during the subjects' reading. while the subject read, the experimenter markedon a copy of the reading passage the words on which stuttering occurred. during the last two periods of the study, when the additional n o word passage was introduced " together with the familiar 200 word passage, the frequency count was taken, separately for both passages. the tape-recording of the reading was played back later during the same day of the recording. a second frequency count was taken and no reference' was made to the markings on the first copy of the passage. this aspect was considered twice since it is possible that visual cues representing struggle responses, especially those associated with non-vocalized blocks, would be noticed during the· reading when the subject was present. it can be expected} therefore, that the frequency of stuttering is greater when taken during reading as compared with the count taken on the playback of the recording. 6. reading rate measured in words per minute. when the taperecordings were played back during the same day of the readings, theywere timed with a stop-watch. the reading rate was calculated according to the number of words contained in the passage (either'200 or 310 .words). as discussed previously, reading rate is probably the most objective.dimension of stuttering behaviour, since it does not rely on any subjective evaluation made by an observer. the amount of stuttering moments and the severity with which stuttering occurs, will directly influence the time it takes a stutterer to read. ' .. . 7. subjects' severity rating of stuttering for the same day, prior to the assessment-interview. the subjects were asked to rate their stuttering as it was for the same day, prior to the interview. the question was put to them in this way: what was your speech like today before coming here? was it the same as usual, slightly better, much better, slightly worse, much worse? this question was very familiar to them as it followed the same format as the daily schedule questions which they were completing. their answer was rated by the experimenter on the following scale: 1 2 3 4 5 much slightly ' slightly much better better same worse worse this rating was made as it was felt that the interview situation might have been perceived as being tense by some subjects, and the likelihood might have arisen that their speech symptoms, during the interview, might have been worse than the speech produced during the day, before coming to the clinic. at the same time the subjects were asked to comment on the "usualness" or "unusualness" of the day they had just experienced. this question was asked in order to elicit information as to whether anything untoward had occurred that could possibly affect their speech. reliability of experimenter's severity ratings. a month after the completion of the experiment, five judges rated the severity of stuttering from the recordings. the purpose of this was to test the reliability of the experimenter's ratings and the degree of agreement with judges. the judges were qualified speech therapists. recordings representing any two of the five assessmenttydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 196 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 myrtle l. aron interviews for each subject were selected according to a set of random numbers and totalled ninety-two samples. these recordings were removed from the original tape reels, spliced together and placed, according to the randomized order, onto large reels. the instructions to the judges included the following: the tape samples you will hear have been made by persons who regard themselves as stutterers. in each sample the "rainbow" passage by fairbanks is read. in some instances a 2 0 0 word portion of the passage will be read, and in others a 3 1 0 word portion will be read. you are requested to make a rating of the severity of stuttering for each sample. this rating is to be made on a nine-point equal-appearing intervals scale on which a rating of i means "no stuttering", a rating of 2 means "very mild stuttering" and a rating of 9 means "very severe stuttering". a rating of 5 indicates "average severity". the other values on the scale represent equal intervals between 1 and 9. please give only one rating for each sample heard. the judges were requested to consider the same criterion of stuttering speech as did the experimenter. results and discussion comparison of the experimenter's severity ratings of stuttering with those made by five judges. judges rated any two recordings of the reading made by each subject out of a possible five. the judges' ratings were compared with each other and with the experimenter's. the results were inter-correlated where the ratings of the five judges were used, together with the experimenter's three ratings (factors ia, ib and ic referred to above). the correlation matrices depict the judges' and experimenter's ratings for the first and second recordings chosen, and the correlations between the ratings of the two recordings. the inter-correlations were derived from raw scores, means and standard deviations. table i : inter-correlations of the severity ratings of stuttering made by five judges and the experimenter for the first group of recordings heard i 2 judges 3 4 5 e i experimenter e2 e3 i-00 •93 1 00 •85 •91 1 00 , •86 •92 •92 i -00 •92 •94 •91 •90 1 00 •88 •88 •85 •88 •88 •88' 1 00 •90 •92 •85 •88 •89 •91 •96 i -00 •89 •91 •88 •88 •90 •95 < -97 i-00 significance at 1 % level = · 36 the matrices show high correlations for the experimenter's three severity ratings (reading, first and second playback). the correlations range from -95 to -97 for both recordings (table i and 2). the correlations between the experimenter's ratings and those of the judges ranged from · 85 to · 92 for the first recordings heard and from · 86 to · 93 for the second recordings heard. these are higher than the correlations between the two recordings for the individual judges (inter-judge), where they ranged from · 71 to · 87 (table 3), journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationships between measurements of stuttering behaviour 23 table 2 : inter-correlations of the severity ratings of stuttering made by five judges and the experimenter for the second group of recordings heakd judges experimenter i 2 3 ' 4 5 ei e2 e3 •00 •92 1 00 •85 •91 i -00 •92 •95 •92 γοο •93 •91 •90 •93 i 00 •87 •92 •89 •93 •90 γοο •88 •92 •88 •93 •90 •95 i-00 •86 •91 •86 •92 •90 •96 •96 i -00 significance at 1% level = 36 table 3 : inter-correlations of the severity ratings of stuttering made by five judges and the experimenter between the t w o groups of recordings heard second recording 1 2 first recordings judges 3 4 5 experimenter ει e2 e3 1 •82 •82 •78 72 •81 •76 •80 •80 . 2 •83 •87 •80 78 •84 •81 •85 •85 judges • 3 •79 •86 •85 76 •85 •80 •83 •84 • 4 •81 •85 •80 77 •83 •82 •84 •84 • 5 •76 •81 •79 7i •82 •77 •81 •82 exp. . . ei ' '73 •79 • "73 67 •75 •75 •79 •80 . e2 •72 •75 •72 67 •74 •76 •80 •80 • h3 •72 •74 •68 62 •73 •72 •76 •77 significance at 1 % level = · 36 or for the between-recordings for the experimenter, where correlations ranged between -75 to ·8ο (table 3). this last correlation is possibly affected by actual differences in the subjects' stuttering from one period to another—an effect by which the judges could not have been influenced. the correlations between the judges themselves (intra-judge) are all high and show a satisfactory measure of agreement. these findings support the evidence contributed by cullinan et al., where intra-judge reliability coefficients tended to be greater than the inter-judge reliability coefficients, i.e. the judges tend to agree better with themselves than with other judges.5 from the matrices it can be seen that the experimenter obtained very high correlations between her three ratings for each recording—the correlations for the first recording ranges from · 95 to · 97 and for the second from • 95 to · 96. this would indicate that there is little difference between the ratings of the severity of stuttering during the reading while the subject was present and a rating made ten days later from a recording. it appears then that, in this study, the visual cues displayed by the subjects while reading had very little effect on the severity ratings made. it can be postulated that tensions displayed during the actual reading (where stuttering is physically manifest and can be "seen") tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 24ivlyrtle l . aron. are reflected in the mariner of reading itself so that they are auditorily detected in the recordings heard. from the results it would appear that the expeririienter's ratings are highly correlated with each other and are satisfactorily in agreement with the five judges. these ratings can therefore be considered as a reliable measurement of the severity of stuttering. this finding also supports sherman's observation that a single observer is sufficient to make reliable ratings of the severity of stuttering.14 c o m p a r i s o n o f s t u t t e r i n g m e a s u r e m e n t s over five p e r i o d s a correlation of 10 factors (points ia, ib, ic, 2, 3, 4, 5a, 5b, 6, 7, discussed above) to observe the nature of the relationship between the measurements themselves was carried out. the matrices for each period are presented in tables 4-8. the means and standard deviations for the respective periods are included alongside the relevant matrix. the ibm 704, 8k computer of the c.s.i.r., pretoria, was used in order to derive these matrices. although it may have been more convenient to refer to one table representing the five periods, the exact nature of the correlations would not have been evident as the scores would have differed due to trends over the five periods. partial correlations were calculated for each period between the experimenter's severity rating (factor ic) and reading rate (factor 6) eliminating any joint correlation with the frequency count of stuttering moments. this was done in order to establish whether the reading rate or the frequency count influenced the severity ratings made by the experimenter. experimenter's three severity ratings. t h e three ratings are highly correlated extending from · 93 to -98. there is a tendency for the correlations to increase from the first period to the fifth period. there is a steady decline in the means from the first to the fourth period (· 48 to · 42), and a decline in the standard deviations from 2 · 0 to 1-5 which indicates that the spread of the scores narrowed. these results may indicate progressive facility with the use of the scale on the part of the experimenter and/or familiarity of the subjects with the experimental situation. on the other hand the means and the standard deviations are increased during the fifth period. the increased standard deviations are probably due to the greater variability between the subjects during the last period of the study. the increased mean scores may also be due to this variability within the subjects, or to a worsening in the stuttering condition as the medication periods had passed. x the severity ratings correlate very 1 highly with reading rate ranging from • 79 to · 92. the correlations are also high with the two frequency counts which. range from · 46 to · 71. the range of correlations of the experimenter's ratings with the rating made for conversation declines steadily from -71 to -46—the correlations, however, are still significant (significance = · 29). the experimenter's severity ratings correlates comparatively poorly with the subjects' severity ratings of their own speech—ranging from · 37 to · 70. this is to be expected as the standards used by the experimenter and by the subjects must differ due to the element of subjectivity present on the part of the stutterers. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e r elation sh ip s betw een m easu rem en ts of stu tterin g b eh aviou r 25 a 3 to'« ν a '••ft; 1 « 3ft; a; a; & c & to to ft; & to i "<3 λ α ft* * ^ pj α w ο 'ίο ν ο ο ο 00 t> ο ο "λν ο σ\ν h ο σ\ ο ο ο ή μ ν μ ο μ \0 ν ο η η μ w ο ο ̂ (j> ο ο̂ νι ο πί ο ο q \ t>00 νι ο πι ο ο (j) ' ο ""φοο πί ο t> ν ο fln σ\00 ο ν ο ν ν ν ο oco hco0o ν ^ 0*ΰ νίν η ο ν ο ο ο̂ \ο ν-»\ο \ο σ\ ο μ ο ο ο ό ό ο t> μ "̂-οο ν 00 ο "λ ο m ν-> ο ο ^ ν οο η ο owo \0 ιλί̂ολο 00 ο η 0 ό ό 6 ό ό |)|) 8 γο ο vo 00 ο̂ \0 ν μ (s m m θ\ m ο σ\ ο ο α\ α\\ο \ο w »rnnoo ο η ο ό ό ο ό ό ό π c? χ) ϋ λ χ) * η η η μ ffitiri v"»\0 t> λ 4-j & c ο c ω c •3 3 bo c τ3 c ω c ο ^ (u -β « s 2 ο. 6 « ο (u « s 2,9 λ qj ο _ £ * β bp £ co m̂ o οοό η ν h vi μ 00 σ\00 00 ν moo t> 00 q\ q\ q\ ο ο vi00 \θ \0 hh tj-00 v) vi f-λο ο ο "φ α\θ moo σ\οο 00 t> 00 00 00 vlosvim v» οί μ t ydskrif van die suid-a frikaan se l ogopediese v eren igin g, v ol. 14, n r. 1: sept. 1967 reproduced by sabinet gateway under licence granted by the publisher (dated 2012) 26 yrtle l . a ron « ο -§ gp co's ο 1 « oi υ υ co i ι--a a q vl ο ο ο ο ν ο ν ο m ο m σ\ ονο tn ο tμ ?ϊ ο 00 μ μ ο σ\\ο m ο ?? ο ο̂ σ\ μ m ο m ν-) ο t-« ο ν ν μ ?? ο t--\0 νο ο ο μ μ ν ο ο f» ό ό ό ^ ζ ό κ ν cfl α « £ ο t-t-υ-ν ρπ ttο ο ο t--00 v -) ό ο ν ν μ fov£> tfvo so 00 co ο ο ο ο ο ο ο < λ £ ) ο «β £ > * η η η ν fotfin îivd t"-[ο ω c •3 3 .6 0 c t3 •ο c 60 c n ·.u μ i s ô oj m g s's 2 o£ x .2 ω s jj u sb o j3 ,ο ελ c .2 js — (u u η a ° lu υ __ « « c "s copl, q μ t- μ ιλ^γ<ίν foo »/-ϊ\0 0 πί tf t-"ψ σ\ 00 f» t̂-00 μ μ μ μ tt "jj i> m μ μ μ ν ν osoo on ο m m e α ν \d \d ν \d ροοο ο \d 00 on vtoo m ο on σ\ «ηνο w νο 00 ο μ ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ο ce χ υ & χ * μ η η ν fo ttin »/ί\0 t·» a jo ω c •3 3 ω c •d c ω c ϋ a \θ c 0> js g ω δ s b ,ο ελ c •3.2 j s ο u t! s « c'? ω ϋ copl, «ο q t--00 ν ο ν 'tonfohoo ν ο t-on t-t-t"» t--00 ffltt-n μ \d t-« •g co μ μ μ μ μ ν τί-τί-ο fo fo 0 e α u int̂ h oooo m m « η m «τί o\ tf t--'m 00 t·»t-m tt tt m oo tt tt t--\0 ν on tf tl-tf tt ιλ ιλ f(1 m \d m m 0 ν ο «ο 5 tn x u ca x μ μ μ ν m ttv) tt ydskrif van die suid-a fkaan se l ogopediese v eren igin g, v ol. 14, n r. 1: sept. 1967 reproduced by sabinet gateway under licence granted by the publisher (dated 2012) 28 ""m yrtle l . 'a ron fl? to'a ts ο α iv (a ο c k & co υ ο co i ο in £ 1' a ο " ο ro ο . ο μ ο m ο m m ο λ ο tt ο ο oo os ο o m ν ο ο νο ο ?? ο «ηοο ο en ο ό tj-vo ο ο μ μ m ο γ ο 'to ο ν ο η ν ν (fin ο t"ό ό «ο μ ο 00 00 vo ν t*-· μ ο ν ν m fovo 00 ο ο ο fo fo 't η μ ο ο ο ό οοο ν ν ν h h n ο on η fv osoo ν ό ο 't^o tf in ιηοο ο ο ο ο ο ο ?? ο osvo ^ μ >/ιοο μ ό ο ό ό ό tfo tr\ fovo ο c \ in in in ιηοο ο ο ο (j) (j) ο η ιλ o\ η ο in tf ο ό μ fovo ό ιηοο οο μ ό ό ό ό ό ο ό τ ϊ £ < & £} ο α χ * ι μ μ μ ν fo ^ ό ιπό t> λ •μ & c ο c ;s ω c '•τΐ 3 ω c τ3 τ3 c ω .3 4-* a jj ~ λ s χ ο <υ is η ο υ ω ,ο £λ c •λ ο <υ η « u υ s « c 'ρ μ ϋ • —ι c3 con, β q "β co os t-ο 00 f--̂οο 00 >λν oooovovovooo γ-ν οο ν tt -̂-ό ν μ ό 00 ν 00 t-m ν ό ο •̂•ososfom moo »nospnt--pnooovo ν ν η η η η ν η 00 γ -"·ν ν ο c s χ > υ c 3 £ > μ μ μ ν journ al of the south a frican l ogopedic society v ol 14, n o. 1: septem ber 1967 reproduced by sabinet gateway under licence granted by the publisher (dated 2012) t h e r elation sh ip s betw een m easu rem en ts of . stu tterin g b eh aviou r 29 to'a <3 ο o iv ft; u ft ο fti co u ο co i e j i -t3 rt ο in £ 1' a. <2 ό κ ν a ο « £ ο ο ο μ ο μ ο 00 ο m t·» ο ο ο μ on ο ο -φ γο ο νο ο ο ^ on -φ ο ο ο ν ν ο η η ν m ο μ νο 00 00 μ ο νο ν ο r -φ -φ m ο γο ν ν γon ο on ν ffν ν ο ο η η η tfn /-100 ν η ό ό ο,ό ό ο ο ο ο α χ > υ α £ > * μ μ μ μ " η ν̂ νο f-λ •μ & c ο jo bc c '•β 3 b£i c t3 c b£i c <υ ω <υ a & ο ^ g « th ,ο £λ <« c •71 o q ό too os μ ν m μ ν os ο m ο os t-m ιτ,ο ο μ in ό \o moo m oo h· "β •>4 co h m h h n n n fflm o m m t s 0 11-ο ν 0 0 0 m hin t-ν t--1-ο tn m μ ν m >noo 00 1λ>λθ on •t't't't't't'n 'n m n ν ν μη μ v. £ «χ ) υ λ μ μ μ ν (fitfin in ό t-cop* t ydskrif van die suid-a fkaan se l ogopediese v eren igin g, v ol. 14,n r. 1: sept. 1967 reproduced by sabinet gateway under licence granted by the publisher (dated 2012) yrtle l. aron the partial correlations calculated for each period between the experimenter's severity rating and the reading rate, excluding the joint correlation with the frequency count, indicate that the actual speed of reading is an important element in the severity ratings made. the partial correlations are all high extending from · 72 to -88. thus it appears that the experimenter's severity ratings are most highly correlated with the reading rate and then with the frequency counts of stuttering moments. these latter two factors are regarded in the literature as being fairly accurate measures of stuttering behaviour, and the satisfactory correlation with the experimenter's severity ratings indicates the relative closeness between these three forms of stuttering measurement. experimenter's severity ratings of stuttering based on conversation. as pointed out the correlations between the ratings based on conversation and on the reading of the passage decline steadily to the fourth period, whereas the means of ratings based on conversation show no such trend—they rise from 4· 6 in the first period to 4· 8 in the second and third periods, falling to 4 • 2 in the fourth period, and then rise to 4 · 5 in the fifth period. this was not accompanied by any increase in the standard deviations—on the contrary, they declined from 2 · 0 to 1-7. it is possible, that because no set amount of conversation (or topic) was required from each subject, the choice of topic, the manner of expression, and the amount spoken might have influenced the experimenter from period to period. in this regard it would obviously have been better to set the subjects a particular topic of conversation during each period, which could be recorded, transcribed from the recordings, and then rated for severity. this is what johnson did with his " j o b " task where the subjects were asked to talk about aspects of their employment.7 subjects' severity ratings of their own speech on reading. t h e correlations between the subjects' ratings of their speech on reading and that based on the recording for the five periods are: -49, -71, -78, -71, -76 (all significant). the low correlation in the first period was probably due to the fact that that the subjects were not at that stage familiar with the routine of the experiment and rating scale. the correlations of the subjects' ratings and the experimenter's ratings of severity are also comparatively low with a wide range from · 37 to · 70. the role of subjectivity and personal feelings about stuttering cannot be discounted. the correlation with reading rate is low, extending from • 22 (not significant) to -65. an interesting observation is that the correlations of the subjects' ratings based on the recording (factor 4) with, that of reading rate, is always lower than the correlation between the subjects' ratings on their original reading and reading rate. this indicates that the stutterers rated subjectively and were probably influenced by their feelings about their speech, especially when hearing the recordings. the fact that reading rate is an objective measure indicates that the stutterer's feelings about his own performance should be viewed as a separate entity when comparing ratings and assessments of stuttering behaviour. the subjects' severity ratings of their speech on reading and on the recording correlates very poorly with their own ratings journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationships between measurements of stuttering behaviour 31 for their speech for the same day prior to the interview—they range from · 04 to · 32. these results are not surprising since the subjects' speech on reading is a situation not practically comparable with spontaneous speech which could be worse or better than while reading. reading does not constitute "spontaneous propositionality," and further the reading passage was chosen for its lack of emotional content, i.e. reading and spontaneous speech represent two different forms of communication. frequency counts of the moments of stuttering. t h e correlations between the two counts taken on the frequency of stuttered moments are extremely high, ranging from · 995 to 1 · 00. this indicates that there was little discrepancy between the two counts taken for each subject, which implies that any visual cues present in the first rating were at a minimum and had little influence on the frequency of the stuttered moments counted. the correlation between the frequency counts and reading rate rose from —-70 to —-76 between the first and second periods, and then declined steadily to —-64 in the fifth period. this was accompanied by a steady fall in the mean frequency counts from 43 to 25 over the five periods. the reading rate is affected by both the frequency of stuttering and the length of the blocks, and the frequency counts by the number of blocks only. the decreasing correlations, together with the fact that the mean number of frequency counts had fallen to a little more than half the original value, may suggest that the shorter blocks tended to be eliminated. following on from this it can be interpreted that the reading rate, towards the end of the experiment, tended to be influenced more by the length of t i e blocks than by the frequency with which they occurred. this is corroborated by the decline in the partial correlation between the experimenter's severity ratings and reading rate, where the joint correlation with the frequency counts was eliminated—the range was from • 88 to · 72 over the five periods. reading rate. this factor has already been discussed in terms of its correlation with the above eight factors. the partial correlations carried out for eaph period between the experimenter's severity rating (factor ic) and the reading rate (where the joint correlation with the frequency count was eliminated) indicate that the speed of reading influenced the experimenter's severity ratings to a large extent in the first period, but they declined towards the fifth period. the correlations are all high and extend from · 88 to · 72. subject's severity rating for their speech for the same day prior to the interview. this rating bears little relationship to the measurements taken. its inclusion was only to provide a rough indication of whether the ratings made on conversation during the interview corresponded with the subjects' report of their stuttering during the same day of the interview. no relationship was found—the correlations range from · 03 to · 23, none of which is significant. the form of speech exhibited in the clinic situation can be expected to differ from the type of speech produced outside this situation. in addition many extraneous factors could have influenced the subjects to make different assessments from the experimenter, e.g. the stutterers' subjective attitude towards speech, the experimenter's practice in making severity ratings. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 32 yrtle l . aron conclusion it was found in this study that a single investigator'can make valid ratings of the severity of stuttering. this was: supported by the high and significant correlations' between five judges and the experimenter. the extremely high correlations between the experimenter's three severity, ratings of the "same reading for individual subjects taken at different times, would seem to indicate that visual cues displayed by stutterers while reading had little effect on the severity ratings made: it was postulated that tensions, where physical manifestations accompanied stuttering, are reflected in the manner of speech itself, so that it can be detected from tape recordings. frequency of the occurrence of stuttered moments, may not necessarily be an adequate measure of stuttering change and severity. severity ratings of stuttering correlated most highly with reading rate. the frequency counts of the moments of stuttering also, correlated well, but not as. highly. the correlations between the frequency count and reading rate extended frpm • 64 to — •76. from the results it appeared that the reading rate, towards the end of the experimental period, tended to be influenced more by the length of the blocks than by the frequency with which they occurred. it is postulated that if the severity of stuttering undergoes change within the same individual over a period of time, this would be corroborated by change in reading rate, rather than frequency. thus it appears that frequency counts of stuttering, while a fairly reliable measurement in itself, is not a sufficiently reliable measure of stuttering severity when used alone. the frequency with which stuttering occurs is referred to in the literature as representing one measure of the severity of stuttering.3·8·23 it can be generalized from the results here that we should differentiate more clearly between the terms "frequency" and "severity" of stuttering. there is no doubt that the amount of frequency of stuttering will contribute to an impression of its severity, but it would appear that frequency as such should not be regarded as an independent and accurate measure of severity. reading rate, while being an objective measure of stuttering, also correlates highly with severity ratings, and therefore indicates that it is a reliable measure which can be taken of stuttering. however, it must be pointed out that this refers to reading only. to rely on rate of speech from transcriptions of spontaneous speech would be difficult as so many variables can confound the issue, e.g. the natural manner and fluency of expression apart from the stuttering moments, the content of what is expressed. in addition, some comparison of speaking rates with normal speakers using spontaneous speech-would have to be considered. the clinical impression that stutterers are poor judges of their own speech performance was borne out by the results of this study. this implies that we, should accept that different standards would effect the assessment of stuttering behaviour as compared to the assessment made by stutterers themselves. it would be relevant for therapists to consider their own rating of stuttering and what constitutes improvement, and separately the stutterers should make their own ratings as to what improvement they feel they are making. it is clear that journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the relationships between measurements of stuttering behaviour 33 we cannot equate both forms of assessment. while there is still a great deal to learn about stuttering behaviour, there is.an indication that we can go some way now in attempting to assess aspects of: it which have a relatively good measure of reliability. . , summary •the report is concerned with the types of measurements that can be made of stuttering behaviour and their relationships to each other. a review of the different measurements that can be taken was presented and the procedure of the experiment was outlined. it was found "that a single investigator can make reliable judgements of the severity of stuttering. three separate severity ratings on the same reading passage were found to be highly correlated. these severity ratings correlated highly.with reading rate, and to a slightly less extent, but still significant, with frequency counts of the moments of stuttering. ratings on conversational speech of stutterers, and "their own ratings of severity were also considered. it was felt that the therapist, should not attempt to^equate her assessment of stuttering, behaviour and any change towards improvement with those assessments of change that the stutterer himself might make. ' opsomming . die verslag handel oor die verskillende soorte metings wat van hakkelgedrag gedoen kan word, en hul verhouding teenoor mekaar. 'n oorsig van die .verskillende metings wat gedoen kan word, word verstrek, en die prosedure wat ten grondslag van die proefneming. le, word geskets. daar is bevind dat 'n enkele navorsingswerker 'n betroubare oordeel oor die erns van hakkel kan vel. daar is bevind dat daar 'n hoe mate van korrelasie tussen drie afsonderlike ernstigheidsbeoordelings van dieselfde leesgedeelte bestaan. daar was 'n hoe korrelasie tussen hierdie ernstigheidsbeoordelings en die lees-tempo, en, in 'n mindere maar nog steeds betekenisvolle mate, die frekwensietellings op die oomblikke dat daar gehakkel is. beoordelings van konversasie-hakkelaars, en hul eie ernstigheidsbeoordeling is ook in ag geiieem. daar word gemeen dat die terapeut geen poging moet aanwend om haar eie evaluasie van hakkelgedrag en enige verandering in die rigting van verbetering gelyk te stel met die evaluasies van veranderings wat deur die hakkelaar self gedoen word nie. references 1. a r o n , m . l . ( 1 9 6 4 ) : the effects of the combination of trifluoperazine and amylobarbitone on adult stutterers. ph.d. · thesis, university of the witwatersrand, · johannesburg. 2. a r o n , m . l . ( 1 9 6 5 ) : the effects of the combination of trifluoperazine and amylobarbitone on adult stutterers. m e d i c a l p r o c e e d i n g s , 1 1 , 2 2 7 . 3. b l o o d s t e i n , o . ( 1 9 5 9 ) : · a handbook on stuttering for professional workers. national society for crippled children and adults, inc., chicago. 4. boehmler, r. m. (1958): listener responses to non-fluencies'. j. speech hearing research, i, 132. . tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 34 yrtle l . aron 5. cullinan, w. l., prather, ε. m., and williams, d . e. (1963): comparison of procedures for scaling severity of stuttering. j. s p e e c h h e a r i n g r e s e a r c h , 6, 187. 6. fairbanks, g. (1940): voice and articulation drillbook. n e w york: harper and brothers. 7. j o h n s o n , w . ( 1 9 6 1 ) : measurements of oral reading and speaking rate and disfluency of adult male and female stutterers and non-stutterers. j. s p e e c h h e a r i n g disorders, monograph supplement 7, 1. 8. johnson, w . , darley, f. l., and spriestersbach, d . c. (1963): diagnostic methods in speech pathology. n e w york: harper and row. 9. johnson, w . , and knott, j. r. (1937): the distribution of moments of stuttering in successive readings of the same material. j. s p e e c h d i s o r d e r s , 2 , 17. 10. n e w m a n , p . w . ( 1 9 6 3 ) : adaptation performances of individual stutterers: implications for research. j. speech hearing research, 3, 223. 11. q u a r r i n g t o n , b. ( 1 9 5 6 ) : cyclical variation in stuttering frequency and some related form of variation. c a n a d i a n j. p s y c h o l o g y , 10, 179. 12. s a n d e r , ε . k . ( 1 9 6 1 ) : reliability of the iowa speech disfluency test. j. s p e e c h hearing disorders, monograph supplement 7, 21. 13. s h e r m a n , d . ( 1 9 5 2 ) : clinical and experimental use of the iowa scale of severity of stuttering. j. speech hearing disorders, 17, 316. 14. s h e r m a n , d . ( 1 9 5 5 ) : reliability and utility of individual ratings of severity of audible characteristics of stuttering. j. speech hearing disorders, 20, 11. 15. sherman, d . , and lewis, d . (1951): measuring the severity of stuttering. j. speech hearing disorders, 16, 320. 16. sherman, d . , and trotter, w. d . (1956): correlation between two measures of the severity of stuttering. j. speech hearing disorders, 21, 426. 17. sherman, d . , young, m . , and gough, k. (1958): comparison of three measures of the severity of stuttering. proceedings of the iowa academy of science, 65, 381. 18. thurstone, l. l. (1959): the measurement of values. chicago: t h e university of chicago press. 19. van riper, c. (1962): personal communication. 20. van riper, c., and hull, c. j. (1934): the quantitative measurement of the effect of certain situations on stuttering. c h a p t e r 8, i n stuttering in children and adults. ed., johnson, w., minneapolis: university of minnesota press, 1955. 21. w i l l i a m s , d . e . , a n d k e n t , l . r. ( 1 9 5 8 ) : listeners evaluations of speech interruptions. j. speech hearing research, 1, 124. 22. williams, d . e., wark, m . , and minifie, f. d . (1963): ratings of stuttering by audio, visual, and audiovisual cues. j. s p e e c h a n d h e a r i n g r e s e a r c h , 6, 9 1 . 2 3 . y o u n g , m . a . ( 1 9 6 1 ) : predicting ratings of severity of stuttering. j. s p e e c h hearing disorders, monograph supplement 7, 31. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e journal of the south african logopedic society teaching reading to the cerebral palsied child ε. m. harrison johannesburg school and treatment centre for cerebral palsied children it is generally accepted that the normal child, from the age of six years is ready for reading, as a result of the spontaneous accumulation of informal experiences, but the cerebral palsied child, because of brain damage has usually a reduced ability to spontaneously grasp the basic essentials of learning — be it reading, writing or arithmetic. therefore, the building up of the foundations on which learning develops is a primary requirement of the greatest importance, and the teacher must plan a programme of carefully graded experiences and activities to give the child >as solid a foundation as possible. in order to be able to plan a "readiness" programme, the teacher must appreciate and fully understand the cerebral palsy problem, entailing physical handicaps, learning difficulties and emotional disturbances, all of which occur in varying degrees, as no two children are alike. an initial assessment of each child by the doctor, psychologist, speech, occupational and physio therapists is essential, so that with this initial knowledge and a still more detailed assessment gained' as the programme is carried out, special attention is paid co the problems of each individual to enable him to gain the maximum benefit at his own rate of progress, and within his capabilities. this consideration of the developmental level of the child is most important, and the stages of development in the normal child are used as a guide. thus, the period in which each child achieves "readiness" will vary, and there is no set age at which the cerebral palsied child may be said to be ready for reading. if, however, throughout the "reading readiness" programme and the reading scheme, the child is made aware of, and develops a positive attitude towards his abilities and disabilities, with emphasis on the abilities, the confidence gained should ensure that there will be a real desire to read and that his first reader will present no problems. the following are some of the many contributory factors which cause the lack of "reading readiness" in the cerebral palsied child. 1) physical handicaps. 2) lack of experience and background. 3) a seeming lack of initiative to use opportunity even when experience and background are present 4) emotional disturbances. 5) visual and auditory perception and spatial concept difficulties; tactile and kinaesthetic sensory losses; visuo-motor lack; short attention and memory span; distractibility; speech involvement and hearing loss. the reading readiness programme for the nursery and kindergarten groups respectively as this begins in the nursery group in the cerebral palsy school where the ages range from two years, often much time has to be spent in first arousing the child to an awareness of its environment, and stimulating active participation before any activity will be productive. in the nursery group and kindergarten, the objectives of the programme are to develop language and the need to perceive, comprehend, discriminate, remember, perceive relationships, reason and be able to transfer what has been learned in one situation to a similar situation, or to make necessary modifications. the activities of the kindergarten in training perception must emphasize appreciation of spatial relationships, discrimination of figure-background and accurate recall of figure outlines, and the ability to reproduce these. it is at this stage, where pictorial representation of the concrete object is developed that further individual difficulties may be found, such as the inability of the child to reproduce on paper what he sees. the programme must always be graded, augmented and intensified to meet the specific needs of each child. the following are examples of activities to develop perception in the nursery group and kindergarten. nursery group — development of perception a—visual 1.—specific concrete activities sorting blocks, buttons and sticks into boxes. colour training. montossori sense training apparatus. play with plasticine and dough, water play, sand trays, paper mache moulding. (the sense of touch developed through these activities is invaluable to the child with visual perception difficulties, as it is through touch r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society and colour that he learns to discriminate shape) threading beads — putting colours together or grouping the different shapes. 2. finger painting potatoe cutouts. puzzles. the use of these is important. cut a simple picture into pieces, having first discussed the picture with the child', and then let the child put it together again. draw the trunk of a man — the child attaches legs, arms, and ears, and puts in the nose, eyes and mouth. a house is drawn and the child puts in windows and doors, etc. β—spatial concept shallow and deep boxes — objects are taken out and put into these shallow and deep tins are filled with sand. rods of different lengths are arranged. big and small balls are used for play. instructions such as "go far away", "stand near the door", "sit together" etc. give a sense of space. c—auditory soft and 'loud music. walk like elephants — "tramp, tramp" and then like fairies, "pitter patter" — these activities to music. percussion band. the child plays with tins filled with different objects and listens as he shakes. when the teacher shakes the tin, he tries to imitate the sound and names what is in the tin. sounds made by animals and things with which the child is familiar, e.g. ear, water, drum, etc. training in sound sequence is developed by the child listening to sounds and saying which he heard first or last, helped by association with concrete objects placed first and last. for example, when dramatising a story such as the "three bears" — father's gruff voice is heard first, then mother's medium pitched voice and finally baby bear's "teeny-weeny" voice. further development of perception in the kindergarten a—visual 1. filling geometric shapes into spaces (trial and error method should be discouraged and the child should be made to feel the shapes). 2. matching shapes. for the child who finds this difficult, colour-cue is used e.g. all squares blue, circles red, etc. 3. matching shapes drawn on cards; this entails figure-background discrimination. if the child is unable to recognize a shape drawn against a background, then the "space" is coloured to help the shape stand out. 4. matching cards on which patterns made up of strokes, circles, etc. are drawn. 5. puzzles. pictures are discussed, cut up and put together again. (this develops appreciation of the whole made up of the parts). 6. letters of the alphabet assorted — the child does not know them as letters. 7. matching pairs of words and also matching similar words. β—auditory this continues to be developed through singing; listening to sounds, e.g. the motor car passing, click of a typewriter, birds singing, voices in the street. also, the child supplies missing rhyming words, and in addition the following activities are used:— 1. the child "looks, says and listens." he then sorts the pictures into groups of like sounds e.g. jam, lamb, pram, and coat, boat, goat; bee, sea, key. 2. he "looks, says and listens" and picks out the odd picture e.g. sun, run, pan; skip, bill, table. 3. listens to the words sun, sit, sell and rubs, lips, bus and then says in which group he heard the " s " first or last. c—spatial concept this is further developed in the handwork and drawing class as "writing readiness," which is an essential part of "reading readiness." directional training, making of patterns in clay, trays with plasticine, sticks, coloured wool, etc., and finally on paper, develops perception of spatial organization and the child learns to perceive form with understanding. this process of analyzing wholes into parts before putting together again, is essential for the cerebral palsied child' who cannot spontaneously reproduce on paper what he sees. it is dear that for the cerebral palsied child writing entails far more than merely developing motor patterns. thus far, the programme will have been based on concrete and semi-concrete activities in which a great variety of materials were used, the importance of which is stressed by strauss and lehtinen who maintain that materials are constructed to dramatize or concretize a process of skill cflear of everything but the basic essentials and that they provide a crutch until skill and understanding are secure (1. p.137). the child should, by the end of the kindergarten training, be organised in his approach to tasks, be able to match words (which to him are at this stage just shapes), and have developed r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society perception of sounds, and be able to recognize, recall and reproduce concrete shapes. grade i here the reading readiness programme is based on developing finer vnsual amd! auditoryory discrimination through activities which lead to a real understanding of word components. visual discrimination in the drawing and handwork lessons the child continues the directional training e.g. up, down, curve to the right, to the left, slanting down to the left; and further concepts, such as shallow, deep, tall, etc, are developed. squares, triangles, circles, diamonds, ovals, etc. are built up making the child more aware of spatial organization; part whole relationships and visual discrimination is increased. simple representations of e.g. cars, birds, bunnies, flowers, egg-cups, etc. composed of lines, curves, etc. are built up using plasticene, coloured gummed paper, feltex and are finally drawn by the child. for example, when drawing the flower — the child makes a curve to the top, right, left and bottom, makes a circle in the centre and a curved stem. the children love these activities and the following is also thoroughly enjoyed. a pattern of shapes, i.e., circles, squares, triangles, strokes, half-moons etc. is built up on the flannel board, left there for a minute or two, then removed. the child must then reproduce this pattern in his book. the importance of this with regard to retaining a mental picture for later word recognition is obvious. letters of the alphabet — to the child the letters are still merely abstract shapes — are also built up on the flannelgraph and then reproduced. auditory discrimination this is further developed by training the child to listen to first, last and middle sounds. at first, the sounds given have an association such as toot (motor-car), boom (gun), tweet (bird), to help the child remember the sequence, especially when there is an auditory perception difficulty. then abstract sounds with no association are introduced, e.g. s, br, m. other activities are: giving words beginning with the same sound; blending sounds together as " s " and " t " ; " h " and " a " ; separating the sounds when the teacher gives a "double" one as "sw." the pattern game is played but this time the teacher "says" a pattern i.e. she says "circle, square, stroke", etc., and the child draws what he heard the teacher say. the child who can now discriminate, visually and aud'itorally, with understanding and reproduce abstract symbols, is ready to relate sounds to their visual symbols — i.e. ready for reading. reading programme the children are told that they are going to make their own reading books. large unlined scrap books are used and only three or four words written on a page. unlined1 books are used as there is less figure-background confusion than in the ordinary lined books. in the initial stages of the programme only three letter-words are used. word recognition: a varied group of words with pictures are written in the books and flashcards of words and pictures made for each child are used. the words and' pictures are matched with those in the book, then without the book and finally the words are used on flashcards alone. (the use of manipulative apparatus helps to reduce distractibility; increases concentration, enables the child to proceed at his own rate and develops interest and a sense of responsibility as the child works with and looks after his own cards.) artistic bookbinders transvalia buildings 21 stiemen's & mell street braamfontein — phone 44-6584 shop 8 in melle street. how important are your i t journals of speech and hearing disorders -fr "speech" -fr notes you can have these bound in attractive volumes to suit your needs. we specialize in binding your thesis according to your design and colour schemes to insure your success. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society sound recognition. two groups of words are given and the child gives the initial sound for each group. the picture of the words e.g. sun, sit, sip, are drawn and also for run, rat, rib. picture cards are made and the child puts them in their respective "s house" and "r house" — he has to say the word himself and listen for sounds. a third and fourth group of word's are given with the " s " and " r " sounds at the end of the word and the same procedure is followed. sounds have shapes. the words are written next to the pictures in the books, and the child looks for the matching symbols in the given words, and thus discovers the symbol for the sound'. the symbol is drawn on a chart with a picture representing the sound e.g. picture of a snake with the s. the words and pictures are used in the same manner as in "word recognition" described above. the child with visual or auditory perception difficulties is helped to remember the shape of the symbol s because of the association with a picture and appreciation of the fact that the first symbol is the first sound heard. the child with no speech is able to show his ability to recognize symbiols and words by matching them with the pictures. recognition and making of shapes the child at this stage is able to recognize, recall and produce concrete known objects. now he will have to recognize, as well as recall and reproduce, abstract symbols. a concrete association must be formed, not only to help recall and reproduce the symbol, but also to recognize and recall the sound for the symbol. the following example shows how this is achieved. the child has related the sound " b " with the symbol in his reading book. the picture of a bat and a ball is associated with the symbol and the initial sound of "bat" and "ball" is b. when writing the letter, the child says "bat" first, and then he says "ball" and so draws first a straight stroke representing bat and then draws a ball. this method prevents confusion of similar letters and reversals when writing and helps recognition of letters and sound recall. word building: when the child knows the sounds used in the first few groups of words, they are used to complete words. for example, the picture of the sun is drawn and next to it -un, is written. pictures of a rat with -at and bus with bunext to the picture are also used. the missing sounds are supplied by the child. the above procedure as from "word recognition" is repeated till all the consonants are known. i have found that by the time about ten consonants have been learned the child knows vowel sounds as well, and through constantly seeing and saying the words with the vowel in the middle, they have no difficulty in filling in the missing sound when given the picture and the word. when the child can recognize a number of words, a sentence describing a picture is written in the reader, e.g. the cat sits on a box in the sun. the sentence is written on cardboard, cut up into words and the child builds it up again. word building the child now "builds" or spells the words by saying the word and writing the sounds in the sequence they are heard. with the complete understanding of sound sequence the child is able to write any three letter-word. double letter sounds are learnt in the same way as the consonants and vowels i.e. by introducing the double soung e.g. "st" at the beginning and end of the words and then the middle sounds such as "ea," "ee" etc. (words and pictures used.) the story: the child colours in a simple picture and pastes it in his reader, and tells the teacher what to write. the child then "reads" the story. (only known words are used). finally the child chooses a picture, pastes it in his book and writes his own story. he really has achieved a "reader" of his own. in conclusion, i would like to point out that in the cerebral palsy class one often finds such children, as the aphasic child, the hard-of-hearing child, etc., each of whom presents their own particular difficulties and for whom more specially modified programmes must be adapted. this programme evolved at the johannesburg school and treatment centre for cerebral palsied children is based on a long term policy of study and much trial and error. the positive results obtained have encouraged us to continue on these lines, and we hope that this article will be of help to others confronted with the problems of cerebral palsy or other fields in which the above suggestions are applicable and useful. references (1) strauss and lehtinen: "brain injured child. vol 1. with acknowledgement to the principal and management committee of the johannesburg school and treatment centre for cerebral palsied children. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'ν biologies-gefundeerde hipotese in verband met die ontstaan van hakkel i . e . u y s b.a. log (witwatersrand), m.a. (u.p.) senior lektrise: spraakheelkunde, universiteit van pretoria, pretoria. opsomming 'n kritiese evaluasie van verskeie teoriee, s o w e l as navorsingsresultate o p die gebied yan hakkel, dui o p baie l e e m t e s , veral m e t betrekking t o t die geheelb e e l d van die probleem. b e w y s w o r d gelewer dat al die verskillende teoriee alleenlik sekere fasette van die p r o b l e e m verklaar, maar nie almal nie. die grootste o n d u i d e l i k h e i d is veral gevind t e n opsigte van die aanvang van hakkel. 'n studie van sekere biologiese verskynsels, wat spraaken taalgedrag t e n grondslag le, lewer 'n m o o n t l i k e verklaring vir s o m m i g e van hierdie onbeantw o o r d e vrae. hakkel w o r d b e s k o u as 'n versteuring in ritmiese p a t r o o n v o r m i n g as 'n t e m p o r a l e verskynsel. klem w o r d veral gele o p die fisiologiese o n v e r m o e t o t t y d s b e r e k e n i n g i n die uiting van lettergrepe. dit w o r d gesien as 'n onderbreking in die ritmiese vloei van spraak as gevolg van 'n koordinasieafwyking, inteenstelling m e t onderbrekings o p die supramorfemiese vlak (wat as normale spraak aanvaar w o r d ) as gevolg van besluite in verband m e t die o n t w i k k e l i n g van sinne. die h o o p w o r d uitgespreek dat hierdie h i p o t e s e as stimulus tot eksperimente sal dien. summary a critical evaluation of different theories and research data in the field o f stuttering p o i n t s t o a lack o f k n o w l e d g e as regards m a n y of t h e salient aspects, particularly in a holistic approach t o t h e p r o b l e m . different theories a c c o u n t for o n l y certain facets of t h e p r o b l e m , but b y n o means all of t h e m . the onset of stuttering still remains a riddle. a study of certain biological p h e n o m e n a underlying speech and language behaviour c o u l d y i e l d e x p l a n a t i o n s for s o m e of the unanswered questions. stuttering is seen as a disturbance in r h y t h m i c patterning, w h i c h is a temporal p h e n o m e n o n . the physiological inability for timing syllable utterance is stressed. this is seen as an interruption in the r h y t h m i c flow of speech d u e t o a disturbance in co-ordination, whereas interruptions at t h e supramorphemic level (which are a c c e p t e d as normal speech) are due t o decision-making in the generation of sentences. the h o p e is expressed that this h y p o t h e s i s m a y serve as a stimulus for further experimentat i o n . gegewens uit kalamazoo en illinois dui aaarop dat die voorkomssyfer van hakkel oor die afgelope aantal jare drasties afneem. daar kom deesdae amper 25% minder hakkelaars onder die skoolpopulasie voor as omtrent 20 jaar gelede.14 hierdie afname kan waarskynlik toegeskryf word aan die feit dat inligting oor tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 i.c. uys die voorkoming en behandeling van hakkel vandag algemeen bekend is. hakkel word nie vandag meer as 'n slegte, vuil gewoonte beskou nie, maar eerder as 'n kliniese probleem. maar dit is nie net vir die leek wat daar vandag meer kennis en inligting hieroor beskikbaar is nie. noukeurige navorsing het daartoe gelei dat wetenskaplike teoriee vandag op navorsingsresultate gebaseer word en baie van die raaiwerk oor hakkel is reeds geelimineer. die toestand vandag onlangs het van riper (1971) 'n nuwe boek oor „the nature of stuttering"14 die lig laat sien. hierin is 'n weelde van navorsingsresultate tesame met sy eie ondervinding in verband met hakkel saamgevat. hoewel dit nog glad nie die antwoorde op die probleem is nie, lewer 'n deeglike studie van die werk tog 'n baie beter insig op in verband met die aard en probleme van hakkel. hy kom tot die gevolgtrekking dat daar vandag eintlik nog net 'n antwoord op twee vrae gelewer moet word. 1. is daar verskillende oorsake wat lei tot verskillende tipes hakkel? 2. lei die een of ander organies-fisiologiese afwyking tot die aanvang van hakkel? indien wel, is dit moontlik om die ontwikkeling van verdere sekondere simptome, neurotiese neigings ens. volgens die leerteorie te verklaar. laat ons probeer om ons kennis in verband met beide hierdie aspekte te ontleed. verskillende oorsake hoewel van riper1 5 hom nie daaraan skuldig maak nie, wil ek graag hakkelgedrag tipeer in 'n poging om verdere riglyne te skep. van riper1 4 stel 4 spore van ontwikkeling voor waar die hakkel by die aanvang, sowel as tydens die ontwikkeling, duidelik van mekaar verskil. lste spoor dit kom voor by ± 50$ van alle hakkelaars. aanvang hakkel begin op die ouderdom van 2mi 4 jaar. vlotheid het voorheen voorgekom; dit begin geleidelik; die hakkel kom periodies voor; spraakspoed is normaal; lettergreep-herhalings sonder spanning of tremor kom voor; hakkel kom op die eerste woorde van die sin en op funksiewoorde voor; hy is onbewus van en nie gefrustreerd oor die onvlotheid nie. ontwikkeling herhalings neem toe; spoed word onreelmatig; herhalings gaan oor in verlengings; spanning en tremor kom voor en dit lei tot frustrasie, vrees en vermyding. dit klink asof hierdie tipe ooreenstem met die'idee van aangeleerde gedrag, waar die kind moontlik gediagnoseer is en die semantogeniese invloede gelei het tot die ontwikkeling van hakkel (verg. johnson). journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η biologies-gefundeerde hipotese in verband met die ontstaan van hakkel 49 2de spoor dit omvat ± 25% van alle hakkelaars. aanvang hakkel begin laat; vlotheid kom nooit voor nie; dit begin geleidelik; artikulasie is swak; spraakspoed is vinnig (uitbarstings); hersiening, gapings en herhalings kom voor sonder tremor of spanning; hakkel kom meestal op eerste woorde, inhoudswoorde en langwoorde deur die hele sin voor; die patroon varieer; die spraak is gefragmenteer selfs tydens vlot periodes; geen bewustheid, frustrasie of vrees kom voor nie. ontwikkeling spraakspoed neem toe; min verandering tree in; duur van onvlotheid neem toe; soms kom vrees vir situasies voor (nie vir woorde en klanke nie); oog-kontak is goed; hy steek nie die hakkel weg nie; hakkel bly primer herhalend; spraak is ongeorganiseerd. hierdie tipe stem baie duidelik ooreen met stamel omdat die idee van ongeorganiseerdheid, tesame met vinnige herhalende tipe spraak waarvan die spreker redelik onbewus bly, sinvol gekoppel kan word aan die beeld van die stamelaar. dit kan ook verklaar waarom daar nie tydens die ontwikkeling 'n merkbare toename in die erns van die probleem is nie (verg. weiss16). 3de spoor dit omvat ± \2% van alle hakkelaars. aanvang die hakkel kan op enige stadium na die aanname van taal begin; vlotheid het voorheen voorgekom; begin dikwels na 'n skielike trouma; artikulasie is normaal; spraakspoed is stadig en versigtig; stemlose verlengings kom voor; laringeale afbrekings kom voor; daar is baie spanning en tremors; die patroon bly konstant; normale spraak is vlot; hy is baie bewus van die probleem, is bevrees en gefrustreerd. ontwikkeling toename in frekwensie en frustrasie; begin spraakpogings oor; fiksasies van orale strukture en verlengings en bewing kom voor; worstelreaksies word waargeneem; spraakspoed neem af;hy weier om te praat en vermy; oogkontak is swak; stemlose toniese afbrekings met veelvuldige sluitings kom voor. die simptomatologiese beeld vanaf die aanvang by hierdie tipe herinner baie aan die een of ander organiese probleem, meer spesifiek 'n moontlike betrokkenheid van die ekstrapiramidale bane (insluitend die corpus striatum) wat kan lei tot 'n vorm van disartrie. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 i.c. uys 4de spoor omvat ±12% van alle hakkelaars. aanvang hakkel begin laat gewoonlik na 4 jaar; vlotheid het voorheen voorgekom; die aanvang is skielik; artikulasie is normaal; spanning en tremor kom voor; hy hakkel op inhoudswoorde; die patroon is konstant; hy is bewus van die probleem, maar nie gefrustreerd nie. ontwikkeling hakkel neem toe; dit is monosimptomaties en simbolies; woordvrese kom nie voor nie en vermyding is min; hy is baie bewus van die probleem, maar hakkel openlik; oogkontak bly goed en hy praat baie; patroon bly oor die algemeen baie stabiel. „they watch you as you react to it. and they are rarely emotional about i t " . 1 4 as gevolg van die kenmerkende laat aanvang, en die onderskeidende simptome, veral die feit dat hy bewus is van, maar nie negatief op die simptome reageer nie, klink dit na 'n tipe hakkel wat sal inpas by 'n neurotiese teorie. (verg. barbara1). aangesien die idee van hierdie 4 spore van ontwikkeling gegrond is op 'n deeglike studie van talle hakkelaars oor baie jare, is dit aanvaarbaar. die groot leemte van 'n teorie gebaseer op verskillende oorsake van verskillende tipes hakkel, le egter opgesluit in die gebrek aan verklaring van alle verskillende fasette van die hakkelprobleem. ons kan egter nie een van die teoriee summier verwerp nie, omdat daar, wat sekere aspekte betref, 'n waardevolle bydrae deur elk gelewer is. 'nevaluasie van die teoriee ν. α. v. navorsingsresultate hakkel as aangeleerde gedrag hakkel word vandag deur meeste gedragsielkundiges soos volg gesien:7 die oorspronklike hakkelmomente word geproduseer deur 'n onaangename emosionele ontwrigting en die hakkel verteenwoordig onderbrekings en disorganisasie van normale spraak.6 dit vind plaas a.g.v. klassieke kondisionering (respondent learning) wat verbind kan word met die werking van die outonome senuweestelsel. operante kondisionering tree in werking (deur die sentrale senuweestelsel) in die ontwikkeling van sekondere simptome. daar is egter 'n paar stellings in verband met hakkel as aangeleerde gedrag wat nadere beskouing verg: 1. hakkel ontstaan onder toestande van negatiewe emosie. hoewel navorsingsresultate nog onvoldoende is, bewys studies dat dit baie selde die geval is. 2. negatiewe emosie vererger normale onvlotheid. navorsingsresultate is tot so 'n mate weersprekend dat geen geldige afleidings daarop gebaseer kan word nie. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η biologies-gefundeerde hipotese in verband met die ontstaan van hakkel 51 3. hakkel kan gekoppel word aan voorafgaande stimuli. ons kan alleenlik aflei dat verwagting in die geval van volwassenes wel 'n rol speel.1 7 4. straf kan hakkel laat afneem of toeneem. redelik uitvoerige navorsing bewys dat beide moontlik is. 5. positiewe versterking laat hakkel toeneem. daar is baie min bewyse om hierdie hipotese te ondersteun. 6. negatiewe versterking laat hakkel toeneem. dit is wel bewys. 7. hakkel is selfversterkend. die vreesvermindering wat voorkom tydens hakkel, versterk moontlik die gedrag, maar geen besliste bewyse is nog gevind nie. hoewel ons dus op hierdie stadium kan se dat klassieke en operante kondisionering sekere aspekte van hakkelgedrag verklaar, vereis die kerngedrag (veral die aanvang) 'n ander verduideliking. die leerteorie verklaar redelik bevredigend die ontwikkeling van hakkel, maar glad nie die ontstaan nie. hakkel as die sekondere gevolg van stamel volgens weiss (1964) is stamel „ . . . . the mother lode of stuttering".16 hierdie teorie word gegrond op twee verskynsels. i. in meeste gevalle word hakkel voorafgegaan deur 'n periode van maklike herhalings, verlengings, huiwerings en selfkorreksies. wanneer dit gepaardgaan met 'n sterk aanduiding van oorerwing, beskou freund8 dit as stamel. dit spruit daaruit dat die dryfkrag tot spraak groter is as die kapasiteit tot ekspressie. ii. nadat die hakkelaar gerehabiliteer is, bly daar nog sekere onvlothede in sy spraak oor, wat weer eens ooreenstem met en dus beskou kan word as stamel. een van die sterkste punte van hierdie teorie is die feit dat stromsta bewys het dat die normale onvlothede van die jong kind wat later begin hakkel het, wel verskil van die kind wat nie in 'n hakkelaar ontwikkel het nie. die moontlikheid bestaan dus wel dat daardie simptome as stamel beskou kan word. ongelukkig verklaar hierdie teorie nie die ontstaan van alle vorms van hakkel nie.8 nog 'n leemte is dat hierdie teorie 'n sekere persoonlikheidstipe aanvaar wat daartoe sal lei dat sekere stamelaars in hakkelaars sal verander.8,9 navorsingsbewyse is nog nodig voor hierdie hipotese aanvaar kan word. hakkel as 'n organiese afwyking daar is iets in die indruk wat die hakkelaar skep wat mens laat wonder of daar nie tog die een of ander organiese probleem is nie. navorsing i.v.m. lateraliteit, genetiese faktore, die kardiovaskulere stelsel, metabolisme, die senuweestelsel ens., lewer in so 'n mate positiewe resultate op dat dit lyk "asof 'n organiese teorie tog nie verwerp kan word nie. onlangse navorsing dui veral op die volgende moontlikhede: 1. dat onvolledige serebrale dominansie of bilaterale spraakverteenwoordiging tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 i.c. uys in verband staan met hakkel (verwys jones2); 2. dat hakkel wel gekoppel kan word aan 'n familiele probleem; 3. dat daar 'n verband is tussen hakkel en allergiese toestande; 4. dat asemhalingsversteurings en spraakspoedversteurings nie net die gevolg van hakkel is nie; 5. dat daar aanduidings van 'n minimale versteuring in die sentrale senuweestelsel by hakkel teenwoordig is. die grootste leemte op hierdie gebied is egter myns insiens die feit dat 'n massa gegewens oor beperkte aspekte van die omvattende probleem ons visie in verband met die totale beeld vertroebel. in teenstelling hiermee is dit egter weer moontlik om volgens die neurokubernetika baie van die ander teoriee te omskryf en te verklaar. so bv. kan sheeh a n 1 2 se teorie i.v.m. die toenaderingsen vermydingsdryfkragte verklaar word volgens elektromiografiese bevindings i.v.m. die ooreenstemming van bewegingsfases en spierspanning. 'nbiologiese grondslag in 'n poging om 'n organies-fisiologiese basis vir die aanvang en kerngedrag van hakkel te soek, is dit miskien sinvol om te begin by 'n hipotetiese teorie wat op grond van bekende biologiese feite verdedigbaar is. as uitgangspunt kan die volgende hipotese gestel word: dat hakkel primer 'n afwyking in die temporale aspekte van spraak is a.g.v. 'n biologiese onvermoe om die motoriese opeenvolging van gegewe klanke, lettergrepe of woorde op die aanvaarde of vereiste momente in tyd te bemeester. motivering en verdediging die voordeel van 'n definisie van hakkelgedrag is, dat 1. die basiese aard van die probleem waargeneem kan word; 2. die aantal afbrekings getel kan word om die erns vas te stel; 3. die gedrag wat gemodifiseer moet word, vasgepen kan word. dit is aanvaarbaar dat daar graadverskille bestaan tussen die onvlotheid van nie-hakkelaars en hakkelaars. navorsingsresultate dui op die volgende:14·2 1. hakkelaars toon meer intrawoord-onvlothede. 2. hakkelaars toon meer verlengings van klanke en herhalings van lettergrepe en woorde. / 3. van alle onvlothede is woorddeel-herhalings makliker diagnoseerbaar as hakkel, terwyl invoegings, hersienings en frase-herhalings eerder as normaal beskou word. ' • 4. die erns van die hakkel word bepaal deur hoeveelheid a. lettergreepof klankherhalings; ' b. klankverlengings; c. gebreekte woorde; d. ongewone klem of spanning. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η biologies-gefundeerde hipotese in verband met die ontstaan van hakkel 53 na aanleiding van bg. bevindings definieer van riper (1971) hakkel as volg: a stuttering behavior consists of a word improperly patterned in time and the speaker's reaction thereto". 1 4 wanneer 'n hakkelspasma voorkom, word die temporale eenheid van die motoriese patroon verbreek as gevolg van tydsdistorsies in die komponente klanke of lettergrepe waaruit die woord bestaan, bv. herhalings, verlengings, gapings en/of invoegings van ander ontoepaslike gedrag. spraak word beide motories en akoesties in tyd gerangskik die bewegings, klanke en lettergrepe moet in 'n voorgeskrewe volgorde voorkom. wanneer hierdie opeenvolging in spraakuiting merkbaar verbreek word, en dit as onaanvaarbaar bestempel word, kan dit gese word dat die persoon hakkel. hakkel is dus 'n afwyking in die tydsberekening en -besteding. hierby is betrokke 'n temporale verbreking van die gelyktydige en opeenvolgende programmerings van spierbewegings wat 'n voorvereiste is vir die produksie van of een van die woord se geintegreerde klanke, of een van die lettergrepe, of die presiese koppeling van klanke en lettergrepe waaruit die spesifieke motoriese patroon bestaan. dit is in werklikheid die kerngedrag in hakkel. dit is egter nog nodig om ter verdediging van hierdie stellings die volgende vas te stel: 1. die hoeveelheid temporale verbreking; 2. die strukture daarby betrokke; 3. die oorsprong van die asinchronie. alle gedrag is 'n integrale deel van die mens se konstitusie; dit is 'n integrale deel van die organiese geheel; dit is verwant aan struktuur en funksie, waar die een die ekspressie van die ander is. gedrag het dieselfde geskiedenis en oorsprong as bv. liggaamsvorm en fisiologiese funksies. met verwysing na die werke van luria,1 1 om maar een te noem, kan ons aanvaar dat gedrag (en hier meer spesifiek spraak en motoriese koordinasie) gereguleer word deur 'n sentrale beheermeganisme. die vloeiende uitvoering van enige beweging vereis sinergistiese interaksie van 'n aansienlike hoeveelheid spiere. oor die algemeen produseer 'n mens ± 220 lettergrepe per minuut (huiwerings en pouses ingeslote). dit is dus aanneemlik dat van 10 000-15 000 neuromuskulere veranderinge per minuut kan plaasvind. die kwessie van tydsberekening en -besteding in spraakgedrag mag nie onderskat word nie. eksperimente het bewys dat waar 'n melodie as stimulus aangebied word, variasies in toonhoogte, luidheid en kwaliteit geelimineer kan word sonder om herkenning te laat skade ly. die melodie word alleenlik onherkenbaar wanneer die interne temporale verhoudings vernietig word. die essensiele aard van 'n tydspatroon is 'n onderliggende pols of slag d.i. ritme. die ritme onderliggend aan spraak'blyk gebaseer te wees op ritmiese wisselings tussen stadiums of toestande. so bv. is daar stadiums van inisiasie en uitvoering van motoriese patrone (siklusse van aktivering en inhibisie). navorsing dui daarop dat hierdie tipe spraakpatroonvorming in tyd gebaseer is op 'n onderliggende ritmiese metriek: (volgens van riper 1 4 is hierdie navorsingresultate van belang) tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 i.c. uys 1. stetson beskou die lettergreep as die ware basiese eenheid van spraak. dit bepaal in 'n groot mate die spraakritme. die fisiologiese korrelaat is die asempols. hy vind dat, aangesien meeste lettergrepe uit 'n vokaalkern met konsonantgrense of stilte bestaan, die konsonante die lugvloei vertraag of stop. dit veroorsaak hindernisse in die vloeiende sametrekking van die torakswande. na aanleiding hiervan is toe vasgestel dat die asempolse rondom ± 6 per sekonde sentreer. 2. hudgins e.a. vind dat die relatiewe spoed van artikulasiebewegings vir die uitspraak van onsin-lettergrepe soos „ta-ta-ta" sentreer romdom 5,5-7,5 per sekonde. 3. lenneberg vind dat selfs sekere denkprosesse hiermee ooreenstem. wanneer mens in die gedagte so vinnig as moontlik tel, kan 'n spoed van omtrent 6 per sekonde gehandhaaf word. 4. brazier het onlangs interessante bevindings i.v.m. e.e.g.-resultate gepubliseer. hy vind dat daar oor die temporoparietal gedeeltes 'n basiese ritme van 7 siklusse per sekonde voorkom. hy bemerk dat kinders nie spraak en taal aanleer voordat 'n sekere mate van elektrofisiologiese rypheid ontwikkel het nie. hy definieer dit in terme van 'n toename in die frekwensie van hierdie dominante ritme wat met ouderdom vasgestel word. 5. molina e.a. merk op dat met elektriese stimulasie van die tailamus, spraak versnel kan word. hulle vind dat die vinnigste wat die persoon kan praat (in hierdie geval tel) ± 170 millisekondes per telling is. dit stem ooreen met ± 6 per sekonde. 6. black vind in verband met die lee-effek dat daar 'n sekere kritiese vertraging is wat maksimale spraakontwrigting meebring, nl. 2/11 sek., of anders gestel, ±1/6 van 'n sekonde. 4 , 1 3 7. cherry en taylor het in eksperimente met digotiese gehoor ook interessante bevindings gemaak. persone is gevra om sinne te herhaal nadat hulle dit gehoor het. die sein is toe reelmatig tussen die linkeren regteroor gewissel. hulle vind dat indien die oorskakelingspoed redelik stadig is, die persoon genoeg inligting links en regs ontvang om die sin met gemak te herhaal. wanneer die oorskakelingspoed te vinnig is, maak die persoon staat op die dominante oor om sodoende die stimulus van die nie-dominante kant in te vul. hulle beskou die kritiese oorskakelingspoed, wat spraakbegrip en -weergawe die meeste belemmer, as een keer elke 1/6 sekonde. hulle lei dus af dat 'n spraaksein die meeste deur hierdie spoed afgetakel word. 1 0 met inagneming van bogenoemde navorsingsresultate kan die hipotese dus gestel word dat temporale patrone vir spraak, waarop neuromuskulere outomatismes gebaseer is, 'n fisiologiese ritme van veranderinge van toestande openbaar nl. siklusse van ± 6 per sekonde. ritme en hakkel heelwat navorsing is al gedoen in verband met ritme en hakkel, waar 'n positiewe verband vasgestel kon word. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η biologies-gefundeerde hipotese in verband met die ontstaan van hakkel 55 die ritme-effek dit is bewys dat die invoering van 'n definitiewe ritme hakkel verhoed of onderdruk, bv. spraak saam met die tik van 'n metronoom. resultate dui daarop dat 'n onreelmatige tik nie die effek het om vlotheid te versterk nie. hoewel ek geen definitiewe aanduidings in die literatuur kon vind nie, behoort dit interessant te wees om te weet waiter metronoomspoed vlotheid maksimaal sal stimuleer. indien in ag geneem word dat hierdie tipe stimulasie tydsberekening van motoriese patrone fasiliteer, kan dit nie summier verwerp word as 'n afleidingsmeganisme nie. van riper dui tereg daarop dat oordrag na gebruik van hierdie tegniek swak is, omdat spraak nie reelmatig ritmies is nie. faktore soos emosie, kommunikasietoestande ens. sal 'n invloed op ritmespoed uitoefen. tydsberekeningsmeganismes in hakkel kliniese waarneming bewys dat tydsberekening 'n belangrike faktor is in die voorkoms van hakkel. so bv.'belnvloed tydsdruk die hakkelaar se spraak. verskeie van die hakkelaar se simptome kan geinterpreteer word as soekende gedrag — gedrag wat daarop gerig is om die regte tydsberekening vir die uiting van 'n woord te bewerkstellig. wanneer sekondere simptome soos kopruk, spiersametrekkings ens. ondersoek word, sal gevind word dat dit ballistiese bewegings is wat die ontsnapping uit 'n toestand van fiksasie bewerkstellig. weer eens is met elektromiografiese navorsing gevind dat tydsberekening 'n belangrike rol speel.1 4 alleenlik wanneer hierdie skielike beweging uit fase is met die tremor, sal ontsnapping moontlik wees. terapeutiese tydsberekeningsmetodes verskeie metodes wat al met sukses aangewend is in terapie, kan beskou word as metodes waardeur die hakkelaar gehelp word met tydsberekening vir sy spraakpogings. hier kan genoem word, froeschels se koumetode, travis en bryngelson se metode waardeur die hakkelaar aangemoedig word om te praat terwyl hy skryf en selfs johnson se bonsmetode. koorlees en skaduspraak wanneer die terapeut en die hakkelaar saam lees dien die terapeut se spraakuiting as bykomende tydsein waardeur tydsberekening in die geval van die hakkelaar se uiting gefasiliteer sal word. min of meer dieselfde proses vind plaas in die geval van skaduspraak waar die terapeut eers die woord se en die hakkelaar dit dan vlot herhaal. hy het weer eens vooraf 'n voorbeeld van tydsberekening en spraakritme gekry. 5 · 1 4 sang nadoleczny, johnson e.a. vind dat minder as 10% hakkelaars onvlot is tydens sang. wingate maak die stelling dat, in teenstelling met metronoomspraak, dit nie die gereeldheid van ritmiese poise in sang is wat tot vlotheid lei nie, maar eerder 'n faktor betrokke by tydsberekening en intonasie. ons weet ook dat daar 'n baie noue verwantskap tussen intonasiekontoere en spraakritme bestaan.1 4 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 i.c. uys adaptasie agnello vind dat hakkelaars in die besonder probleme ondervind met die oorgange wat nodig is in die uiting van snelseers (tongue twisters). in verband hiermee merk wingate op dat by die herhaalde lees van dieselfde stuk, die hakkelaar 'n verbetering toon in die tydsberekening vir motoriese koordinasiepatrone. ter bevestiging van hierdie stelling, vind besozzi en adams3dat die adaptasie-effek duideliker waarneembaar is wanneer die persoon die stuk elke keer hardop lees. adaptasie neem af wanneer stillees soms toegelaat word. neurologiese ritme lindsley en stromsta1 4, vind dat bilaterale e.e.g.-opnames van nie-hakkelaars toon 'n groter mate van gesinchroniseerdheid as die van hakkelaars. elektromiografiese studies deur travis14 dui weer aan dat aksiepotensiale van die maseterspiere bilaterale wanverhoudirigs in aankomstye tydens hakkel toon, wat nie voorkom tydens vlot spraak nie. studies deur chevrie-muller lewer ook bewys van asinchronie in stemlipfunksie by hakkelaars.14 hoewel hierdie studies nog nie afdoende bewyse van al die verskillende fasette lewer nie, mag die aanduidings daaruit verkry nie onderskat word nie. afleiding om terug te keer tot van riper 1 4 se definisie van hakkel, m.b.t. „ a word improperly patterned in time ".is daar sekere feite aan die lig gebring wat daarop dui dat dit eerder 'n geval is van „ . . . a syllable improperly patterned in time " wanneer verwys word na studies i.v.m. die diagnose van hakkel deur luisteraars, is dit duidelik dat die sterkste gereageer word op verbreking van die lettergreep. „not all of the interruptions to the forward flow of speech are signs of stuttering". „ of all the types of disfluency, part word repetitions are more likely to be classified by listeners as 'stuttering' and that certain other kinds of disfluency, most notably perhaps interjections, revisions, and phrase repetitions, are more commonly considered as 'normal' disfluencies". 'n verklaring hiervoor kan gevind word in 'n studie van die aanname van spraak en taal by die kind, normale onvlotheid en die aanvang van hakkel. volgens van riper 1 4 leer die kind woorde as motoriese eenheidspatrone. dit , is miskien meer korrek om te se dat die kind spraak op die morfemiese vlak as eenheidspatrone aanneem. elke opeenvolgende benadering word gemodifiseer tot die produk kommunikatief aanvaarbaar is, m.a.w., bekende motoriese patrone word omvorm en gefatsoeneer (shaped) en dit vereis terugvoering. hierdie patrone word dan gestabiliseer, afhangende van oefening, terugvoering en die kompleksiteit van die koordinasie. van riper 1 4 meen dat stabiliteit in 'n hierargiese volgorde verdeel kan word nl. die. klankvlak, lettergreepvlak, woordvlak en sinvlak, waar die klankvlak die meeste stabiel sal wees en die sinvlak die minste stabiel. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η biologies-gefundeerde hipotese in verband met die ontstaan van hakkel 57 weer eens kan daarop gedui word dat die morfemiese vlak meer stabiel sal wees as die supramorfemiese vlak. meeste woorde val buite die grense van die morfemiese vlak. dus kan meeste woorde ook met reg beskou word as supramorfemiese strukture, tesame met die frase en die sin, omdat dit gedurig gegenereer word. in die normale onvlotheidsperiode kom afbrekings op die supramorfemiese vlak dikwels voor. hierdie ongeorganiseerde vorm van spraak word meestal deur luisteraars as aanneembaar beskou in die spraak van kinders, en volwassenes, juis omdat dit 'n funksie van besluite is wat nog geneem moet word. gedisorganiseerde spraak kan herken word aan afbrekings op die morfemiese vlak, waar hierdie afbrekings 'n funksie is, nie van besluite nie, maar van temporale motoriese koordinasie. dit is juis omdat die lettergreep die basiese fisiologiese eenheid van spraak is. spanning, emosionele reaksies en tydsdruk ens., sal dus die opeenvolging van spraak verbreek op die mees basiese vlak van integrasie die morfemiese vlak, of meer spesifiek, lettergreepvlak. emosionele druk sal die asempols versteur sodat tydsberekening vir die uiting van lettergrepe sal skade ly. hierdie onderbrekings op die lettergreepvlak word waarskynlik deur meeste outoriteite as woordonderbrekings gesien, omdat dit die vloei en selfs kommunikasie van die woord aantas. ons kan egter net weer eens verwys na die stelling dat woorddeel onvlothede die vinnigste as hakkel gediagnoseer word. op enige wetenskaplike gebied word gereeld verklarings gemaak en verduidelikings gelewer i.v.m. waarneembare gebeurtenisse. hierdie verduidelikings word gegrond op 'n verwysingsraamwerk. op die gebied van hakkel is daar egter twee faktore wat verduideliking en verklaring strem. 1, 'n massa navorsing i.v.m. sekere fasette van hakkel vertroebel en strem ons siening van die geheelbeeld. 2. omdat die aanvang en oorsprong van hakkel nie tot dusver vasgepen kon word nie, is navorsers geneig om daarvan weg te skram en hulle eerder toe te le op die aangebode simptomatologiese beeld wat hom makliker tot verklaring leen. die formulering van verduidelikings het egter al dikwels as stimulus tot eksperimentering gelei. miskien kan hierdie hipotese tog bydra tot 'n beter insig in die probleem van hakkel as 'n geheel: hakkel ontstaan as gevolg van 'n biologiese onvermoe om op die morfemiese vlak motoriese patrone temporaal te bemeester. as gevolg van hierdie disorganisasie in spraak, word verskeie ander gedragsvorms omvorm en aangeleer in 'n poging tot aanpassing en kompensasie. dit volg noodwendig dat waar 'n teoretiese orientasie soveel as moontlik verwys na die totaliteit van 'n probleem, die behandeling wat daarop geskoei is, omvattender sal wees en dus 'n groter kans sal he op sukses. bibliografie 1. barbara, d.a. (1965): stuttering: a psychodynamic approach to its tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 i.c. uys understanding and treatment. thomas, springfield, 111, u.s.a. 2. beech, h.r. and fransella, f. (1968): research and experiment in stuttering. pergamon press. oxford. .3. besozzi, t.e. and adams, m.r. (1969): the influence of prosody on stuttering adaptation. j. speech and hearing research, 12, 818-824. 4. black, j.w. (1951): the effects of delayed side-tone upon vocal rate and intensity. j. speech and hearing disorders, 16, 56-60. 5. bloodstein, 0 . (1950): hypothetic conditions under which stuttering is reduced or absent. j. speech and hearing disorders, 15,142-153. 6. bluemel, c.s. (1935): stammering and allied disorders. macmillan, n.y. 7. brutten, e.j. en shoemaker, d.j. (1967): the modification of stuttering. prentice-hall inc. n.y. 8. freund, h. (1966): psychopathology and the problems of stuttering. thomas, springfield, 111. 9. freund, h. (1952): studies in the interrelationship between stuttering and cluttering. folia phoniatrica, 4,146-168. 10. lenneberg, e.h. (1967): biological foundations of language. j. wiley and sons inc., n.y. 11. luria, a.r. (1966): higher cortical functions in man. tavistock. london. 12. sheehan, j. (1958): conflict theory of stuttering, in stuttering: a symposium, j. eisenson, ed., harper, n.y. 13. stromsta, c. (1959): experimental blockage of phonation by distorted side-tone. j. speech and hearing research, 2,286-301. 14. van riper, c. (1971): the nature of stuttering. prentice-hall inc., n.y. 15. van riper, c. (1972): speech correction. 5th edition. prentice-hall inc., n.y. 16. weiss, d.a. (1964): cluttering. prentice-hall inc., n.y. 17. wischner, g.j. (1950): stuttering behavior and learning. j. speech and hearing disorders, 15,324-335. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sajcd 133 speech-perception-in-noise and bilateral spatial abilities in adults with delayed sequential cochlear implantation i oosthuizen, d swanepoel, c van dijk   department of communication pathology, university of pretoria ilze oosthuizen   department of communication pathology, university of pretoria; callier center, school of behavioral & brain sciences, university of texas, dallas, texas, usa; ear science institute australia, subiaco, australia, and ear sciences centre, school of surgery, university of western australia, nedlands, australia de wet swanepoel   department of communication pathology, university of pretoria catherine van dijk corresponding author: d swanepoel (dewet.swanepoel@up.ac.za ) objective. to determine speech-perception-in-noise (with speech and noise spatially distinct and coincident) and bilateral spatial benefits of head-shadow effect, summation, squelch and spatial release of masking in adults with delayed sequential cochlear implants. study design. a cross-sectional one group post-test-only exploratory design was employed. eleven adults (mean age 47 years; range 21 69 years) of the pretoria cochlear implant programme (pcip) in south africa with a bilateral severe-to-profound sensorineural hearing loss were recruited. prerecorded everyday speech sentences of the central institute for the deaf (cid) were used to evaluate participants’ speech-in-noise perception at sentence level. an adaptive procedure was used to determine the signal-to-noise ratio (snr, in db) at which the participant’s speech reception threshold (srt) was achieved. specific calculations were used to estimate bilateral spatial benefit effects. results. a minimal bilateral benefit for speech-in-noise perception was observed with noise directed to the first implant (ci 1) (1.69 db) and in the speech and noise spatial listening condition (0.78 db), but was not statistically significant. the head-shadow effect at 180° was the most robust bilateral spatial benefit. an improvement in speech perception in spatially distinct speech and noise indicates the contribution of the second implant (ci 2) is greater than that of the first implant (ci 1) for bilateral spatial benefit. conclusion. bilateral benefit for delayed sequentially implanted adults is less than previously reported for simultaneous and sequentially implanted adults. delayed sequential implantation benefit seems to relate to the availability of the ear with the most favourable snr. keywords: bilateral benefit, cochlear implant, head-shadow effect, sequential implantation, speech-in-noise perception, squelch, summation s afr j cd 2012;59(1):45-52. doi:10.7196/sajcd.133 cochlear implants (cis) are widely recognised as the most successful sensory prosthetic device in the medical world (wolfe & schafer, 2010). the widespread success has led to bilateral cochlear implantation becoming accepted medical practice in clinically suitable adults and children. the recent bilateral cochlear implantation position statement underscores the importance of bilateral implantation to enhance bilateral processing benefits for users (balkany et al., 2008). the advantages of binaural hearing may include a range of benefits such as improved listening or speech recognition in quiet and in noise, localisation, directional hearing, and bilateral spatial benefits, such as head-shadow effect, summation, and squelch (litovsky et al., 2004; cochlear corporation limited, 2005; litovsky, parkinson, arcaroli & sammeth, 2006; neuman, haravon, sislian & waltzman, 2007). the head-shadow effect is the strongest and most robust bilateral benefit effect demonstrated for spatially separated speech-in-noise tests (laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006). in implanted adults, head-shadow effects of 3 11 db have been demonstrated (gantz et al., 2002; laszig et al., 2004). improved speech understanding in noise can be attributed to a second bilateral spatial benefit effect, namely the binaural summation effect. the latter can improve speech perception scores up to 19% in quiet and up to 16% in noise (tyler et al., 2002). summation effects previously reported vary in effect sizes of up to 6 db in some users and no effect or negative effects in others (gantz et al., 2002; laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006; wolfe et al., 2007). the squelch effect is another possible advantage of bilateral hearing through bilateral cochlear implantation. it is a modest bilateral benefit with effects up to 2 db, although some reports indicate no effect or even negative effects (gantz et al., 2002; laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006). bilateral implantation can be divided into the following categories: (i) simultaneous implantation where both ears are implanted during the same surgical procedure; (ii) sequential implantation with inter-implant intervals between 6 and 12 months on average; and (iii) delayed sequential implantation with inter-implant intervals of more than 2 years (manrique, huarte, valdivieso & pérez, 2007; peters, litovsky, parkinson & lake, 2007). in the south african context simultaneous implantation is not yet routinely employed. of all the patients of the pretoria cochlear implant programme (pcip) only 13% have received bilateral cochlear implants (bicis), all of which were sequentially implanted. the tygerberg programme is the only programme that has three simultaneously implanted adults (a. m. u. muller, personal communication, 17 november 2010). there are several reasons why patients in the south african context receive sequential cis, among others: limited reimbursement or financial resources to fund simultaneous implantation surgery; additional risk and difficulty of extended surgical procedures; the practice of preserving one ear for future technologies; difficulty in obtaining collaboration from medical insurance providers/medical aid funds for simultaneous bilateral implantation; and extended time needed for mapping two implants at the same time, which could lead to fatigue, especially in young children (laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006). there are limited data on bilateral benefits of delayed sequentially implanted adults in previous studies. this might suggest that this population may not benefit from bilateral processing, such as sound localisation and speech-in-noise perception. it is clear from previous studies of bilateral implantation in adults that more robust improvements in speech-in-noise perception are typical of simultaneous-implant ci users (tyler et al., 2002; cochlear corporation limited, 2005; ramsden, et al., 2005; litovsky et al., 2006). galvin and colleagues (2010) aimed to determine if adolescents (≥10 years of age) gained additional perceptual benefit from sequential bilateral cis within 12 months. the results indicated that adolescents may gain additional benefit from a second cochlear implant (ci 2) regarding bilateral perception, even if their hearing loss is congenital and it is more than 16 years since the receipt of their first cochlear implant (ci 1). tyler, dunn, witt and noble (2007) found that sequentially implanted adults received significant bilateral improvement on at least one speech-perception test compared with either implant alone. they concluded that sequential implants can be beneficial for adults even after many years of monaural use and even with very different cis. however, although ramsden et al. (2005) reported a significant bilateral benefit for speech perception in quiet and in noise for sequentially implanted adults, it was concluded that sequential implantation with long delays between resulted in poor second-ear performance for some individuals and has limited the extent of bilateral processing benefit that can be obtained by these users. litovsky et al. (2006) found that all simultaneously implanted patients showed significant bilateral processing benefit on at least one of the speech-perception-in-noise measures, and the strongest bilateral benefit was measured for the head-shadow effect. some individuals with simultaneously implanted cis showed evidence of binaural squelch and summation (litovksy et al., 2006). it is possible that early second cochlear implantation for adults with sequential cochlear implantation might allow better acquisition of bilateral processing and bilateral spatial hearing, thus leading to improved speech-perception-in-noise performance (litovsky et al., 2006). previous studies did not routinely determine which ci was the superior functioning implant (cochlear corporation limited, 2005). it would appear that delayed sequential cochlear implantation could affect the extent of the experience of bilateral processing benefit in terms of speech-in-noise perception, especially spatially coincident speech and noise signals. bilateral implantation for adults in south africa currently occurs predominantly in a delayed sequential time frame. consequently, the question is how well these delayed sequentially implanted users are able to perceive speech in noise. the main aim of this study therefore was to determine the bilateral speech-perception-in-noise benefit in a group of delayed sequentially implanted adults. the following sub-aims were identified in order to attain the main aim: • to determine the speech-perception-in-noise ability (in spatially separated and spatially coincident speech and noise listening conditions) • to calculate the bilateral spatial benefits (head-shadow effect, summation, squelch, and spatial release of masking (srm)) using abovementioned results compared with normative data. materials and methods the study objective was to assess the speech-perception-in-noise abilities (with speech and noise spatially distinct and coincident) as well as bilateral spatial benefits (head-shadow effect, summation, squelch and srm) in adults with delayed sequential cochlear implantation. this study was approved by the institutional review board before any data collection commenced. participants eleven adult patients of the pcip were recruited, with a bilateral severe-to-profound sensorineural hearing loss, sequentially implanted with systems from cochlear™ with at least 1 year of bilateral use and recently mapped and balanced in terms of loudness between the implants. ten participants’ first language was afrikaans. only one participant spoke english as first language. the participant selection criteria are provided in table 1. table 1. selection criteria for participants criterion justification clients with sequential bilateral cochlear implants (cis) all clients should have been sequentially implanted (cis implanted during separate surgeries) (lustig & wackym, 2005) as the main aim of the study is to determine the bilateral processing benefits achieved in sequentially implanted ci users type of ci: all participants should be implanted with nucleus cis from cochlear™ to date, the pretoria cochlear implant programme (pcip) only implants nucleus products. this criterion ensured uniformity of the product, thus lessening variability of the outcomes model of ci: freedom, nucleus 22 or nucleus 24 these three models are products from cochlear™, with which the clients of pcip are implanted. other types of ci models such as double array implants were not included. this helped diminish variability of the outcomes of the study duration of time since implantation: participants must already have been using their second cochlear implant (ci 2) for at least a period of 1 year   the duration of at least 1 year’s use is to ensure that the map for this implant would have been stabilised (hughes et al., 2001). furthermore, the participant must have had time to become adequately adjusted to his/her bilateral cochlear implantation status in terms of wearing and using both devices. according to the literature, adult ci users typically reach their performance plateau within 6 months to 1 year post-implantation (teoh, pisoni & miyamoto, 2004). participants’ cis were required to have been bilaterally balanced with the company’s (cochlear™) software a month before the proposed test battery for data collection it is imperative to ensure even balance of the loudness of both devices, as sounds will lateralise to the louder ear if loudness is unbalanced (cochlear corporation limited, 2005). to remove the influence of binaural loudness summation on performance as far as possible, laszig et al. (2004) suggest that loudness balancing of unilaterally and bilaterally used processor programmes/maps be required aided pure tone thresholds (air conduction) between 25 and 40 db hl and aided speech discrimination scores of ≥70% clients with aided thresholds greater than 40 db hl and aided speech discrimination scores less than 70% may be viewed as not well adapted and thus not good ci users (moore & teagle, 2002) type and degree of hearing loss (prior to implantation): participants were required to have had a bilateral severe-to-profound (71 db hl to > 90 db hl) or moderate-to-profound (41 db hl to > 90 db hl) sensorineural hearing loss (clark, 1981 in harrel, 2002) prior to the implant the participants’ type and degree of hearing loss should correspond with candidacy criteria as accepted by the pcip. these criteria are based on the selection criteria of cochlear™, where bilateral severe-to-profound or moderate-to-profound sensorineural hearing loss is stated as first criterion for adults (cochlear corporation limited, n.d.) participants were required to be clients of the pcip at the university of pretoria     this ensured uniformity among participants. it was logistically more convenient for the researcher to conduct the fieldwork at the pcip as she had access to the premises as well as to clients’ records. the relevant information was therefore easily available and obtainable language: participants should be afrikaans and/or english speaking the participants must be able to participate in the required test battery. the majority of bilaterally implanted clients of the pcip are either english or afrikaans speaking. the researcher is also only proficient in these two languages. this ensured clear communication during informed consent and the course of fieldwork ages: the participants were required to be 18 years or older the study aimed to investigate the adult population of the pcip and this criterion also ensured that informed consent could be obtained from the participants themselves hearing loss aetiology was determined from medical records. participant ages ranged from 21 to 69 years at the time of testing (mean 47 years) with age at first and second switch-on ranging from 4.6 to 61.1 years (mean 37.7 years) and 16.6 to 64.6 years (mean 43.3 years) respectively (table 2). duration between the ci 1 and ci 2’s switch-on ranged from 16 months (1 year 4 months) to 12 years (mean duration 4.3 years). the duration of bilateral implant use ranged from 24 months to 54 months (mean duration: 49 months). most participants (9/11) can be considered as late implanted with unfavourable interval periods (>2 5 years). table 2. description of research participants no. age at test (years) age at hl onset (years) likely hl aetiology ci 1 switchon age (years) ci 1 description ci 2 switchon age (years) ci 2 description ci 1 and ci 2 interval (years) duration of bici use (years) 1 59.5 5 chronic otitis media 51.8 nucleus 24 m freedom 54.1 nucleus 24 ca freedom 2.5 5.4 2 69.10 9 mumps 59.2   nucleus 24m esprit 3g 64.5   nucleus 24ca esprit 3g 5.3 5.5 3 66.8 28 progressive 61.10   freedom 24ca freedom 64.6   freedom 24ca esprit 3g 2.8 2.2 4 66.3 31 progressive 55.9   nucleus 24m freedom 61.7   freedom 24ca freedom 5.10 4.8 5 60.3 35 progressive 45.10   nucleus 22m freedom 56.4   freedom 24ca nucleus 5 10.6 3.11 6 23 0 extreme prematurity & complications 19.5   freedom 24ca nucleus 5 21.11   freedom 24ca freedom 1.4 2 7 21.10 2 meningitis 4.6   nucleus 22 freedom 16.6   nucleus 24ca freedom 12 5.4 8 54.3 13 progressive 47.7   nucleus 24 freedom 49.1   nucleus 24ca esprit 3g 1.6 5.2 9 32.6 0 genetic 23.11   nucleus 24k nucleus 5 29.6   freedom 24ca freedom 4.7 4.1 10 44.6 22 post-traumatic mva 39.8 nucleus 24ca freedom 41.4 freedom 24ca freedom 2.8 3.2 11 20.11 0 congenital 7.1 nucleus 22m esprit 3g 17 freedom 24ca freedom 9.11 3.11 hl = hearing loss; ci 1 = first cochlear implant; ci 2 = second cochlear implant; bici = bilateral cochlear implant. materials and methods an audiometric booth, certified annually, was used to provide a sound-treated environment during testing. this was to ensure accurate and reliable pure tone and speech measurements. speech and noise were presented from separate loudspeakers with an angular separation of 90° between the speakers (figures 1 3). speech was always presented from the front and noise was presented from a different loudspeaker in order to direct the noise to the participant’s right and then his/her left ear. for spatially coincident speech and noise, speech and noise were presented from a single loudspeaker in front of the participant. fig. 1. test set-up to determine speech-perception-in-noise abilities: speech and noise spatially separated with noise directed to the right ear. speech was presented from the front loudspeaker (numbered 2) and noise was presented from the loudspeaker on the participant’s right-hand side (numbered 3).   fig. 2. test set-up to determine speech-perception-in-noise abilities: speech and noise spatially separated with noise directed to the left ear. speech was presented from the front loudspeaker (numbered 3) and noise was presented from the loudspeaker on the participant’s left-hand side (numbered 2). fig. 3. test set-up to determine speech-perception-in-noise abilities: speech and noise spatially coincident. speech and noise were presented simultaneously from the loudspeaker directly in front of the participant (numbered 2). pre-recorded everyday speech sentences of the institute for the deaf (cid) were used to evaluate the participants’ speech-in-noise perception at sentence level (alpiner & mccarthy, 2000). the afrikaans translated version of these sentences, by muller and de stadler (1987) was used for the 10 afrikaans-speaking participants. before presentation of each sentence list, a calibration tone was presented in order for the researcher to monitor the volume unit (vu) meter of the audiometer to ensure that the audiometer presented the recorded speech material at the specified level (wilber, 2002).sentences were presented through the specified loudspeaker as a closed set. participants were instructed to repeat each sentence as it was presented and no feedback as to correct or incorrect response was provided. continuous speech noise was selected, and presented simultaneously with the sentences at a fixed level of 55 db hl. an adaptive procedure was used to determine the signal-to-noise ratio (snr, in db) at which the participant’s speech reception threshold (srt) was achieved, thus the level where the participant achieved at least 50% performance. in this process the first sentence was presented at 0 db snr, i.e. speech and noise presented at 55 db hl. the speech signal level of the first sentence was increased in steps of 2 db until the participant could identify the first sentence correctly, based on the number of correct keywords. subsequently, the remaining sentences were presented adaptively in a one-up, one-down method with a 2 db step size. the test result was the average snr of the last six presentation levels. the measures for spatially separated speech and noise were conducted with the noise directed to the participant’s right ear (nr), with only the ci 1 switched on, then with ci 2 switched on and finally with both implants switched on at once (bici). subsequently the noise was directed to the participant’s left ear (nl) and then together with the speech from the front loudspeaker following the same procedure as for the nr condition. thus, there were nine possible listening configurations. however, results for spatially separated speech and noise were discussed in terms of noise ipsilateral to the ci 1 and noise ipsilateral to the ci 2. to estimate the bilateral spatial benefit, i.e. the effects of head shadow, summation, squelch and srm, the following calculations were used (van deun, van wieringen & wouters, 2010): head shadow 90° – the head-shadow effect arising from a shift in the noise position of 90° – was calculated as the difference in the srt value (in db) obtained with the left/right ear in the noise from the front (nf) versus nr/nl condition. head shadow 180° – this head-shadow effect was calculated as the difference in the srt value (in db) obtained with the left/right ear in the nl/nr versus nr/nl condition when there was a 180° change in the noise position. squelch – this is the enhancement in speech perception owing to the addition of an ear with a poorer snr. it was calculated as the difference between the srt values (in db) for the left/right ear and both implants in the nr/nl condition. summation – summation is produced by binaural redundancy (also known as diotic summation), that is the difference between bilateral and better ear performance in spatially coincident speech and noise (schön, müller & helms, 2002). subsequently, summation was calculated as the difference between the srt values (in db) of the ci 1/ci 2 and bicis in the nf condition. spatial release of masking (srm) – this is the improvement in speech perception as a result of spatial separation of speech and noise when listening with both ears. hence, srm was determined as the difference in bilateral srt values (in db) in the nf versus nr or nl condition. in support of the srm effect, the benefit of adding the better snr ear was determined. this implies the improvement in speech perception resulting from the addition of an ear with a better snr. thus, the difference between the srt value for the left/right ear and the bilateral srt in the nl/nr conditions was determined. this could possibly include all of the abovementioned spatial benefits because an ear is added in a situation with spatially distinct speech and noise. quantitative methods were utilised to analyse and process data electronically by means of a statistical software package (spss) (field, 2005). descriptive comparisons were made and the mann-whitney u-test was used to draw conclusions about the sample population on a 5% level of significance. the mann-whitney u-test is a distribution-free test and was selected because of the small sample size (steyn, smit, du toit & strasheim, 2003; miller & miller, 2004). reliability and validity to increase reliability as far as possible the following steps were implemented: • each participant was contacted personally, telephonically or via electronic mail to explain the purpose of the study to them and to obtain their consent to participate. • a qualified and registered audiologist performed the measurements. • a qualified service technician was involved to verify the use of the correct equipment and test set-up. • sound level measurements of the intensity of the signals to be presented in sound field were done before commencement of the testing procedures to ensure that the signals were presented at the specific intensity for each of the tests. • a specific test set-up was used for each participant with marked places for the participant and speakers, according to recent literature (cochlear corporation limited, 2005). • the clinical audiometer that was used was calibrated to ensure accurate measurements. the calibration standard of the international standards organisation (osi) is accepted in south africa. • recorded cid sentence test material was used for the speech-in-noise tests, to further enhance reliability and to avoid the presenting variability of using live voice. furthermore, the possibility of using speech reading or lip reading by participants to support their speech perception was eliminated by the use of recorded sentence test material which increased the reliability even more. to increase internal and external validity the following strategies were employed. before the test battery was conducted, each participant’s ci was mapped and balanced to ensure optimal functioning for the testing procedures. the same audiologist conducted the same measurements for each participant. all participants received the same information regarding the purpose of the study and their role during the study, as well as identical instructions during the measurements. during the tests, participants used the programme on their processors that they use for general listening in order to obtain a reflection of their everyday functioning. as the pcip only has a small population of sequentially bilateral implanted adults, the purposive convenient sampling method and selection criteria were vigorously implemented to select a representative sample of sequentially implanted adult clients of the pcip (leedy & ormrod, 2005). results and analysis speech-in-noise perception tables 3 and 4 illustrate the snr values for ci 1, ci 2, the superior performing implant and both implants (bici) in the spatially distinct speech and noise conditions and in the spatially coincident speech and noise conditions, respectively. from table 3 it is clear that in the unilateral listening conditions a performance advantage for the ear opposite the noise source was evident in 91% (n=10/11) and 55% (n=6/11) of participants with noise directed to ci 1 and ci 2, respectively. table 4 indicates that ci 2 was the superior performing implant for speech-in-noise perception with speech and noise being coincident for the majority of participants (n=9/11). table 3. snr values for ci 1, ci 2, superior implant and bilateral implant condition in spatially separated speech and noise (n=11)   spin with noise on ci 1   spin with noise on ci 2   ci 1 ci 2 superior ci bici   ci 1 ci 2 superior ci bici 1 24.67 db 17 db ci 2 15 db*   20.33 db 27.6 db ci 1 23 db 2 29 db 28.67 db ci 2 28 db*   28.67 db 28 db ci 2 28.33 db 3 26.67 db 9.67 db ci 2 3.67 db*   21.67 db 17 db ci 2 16.33 db* 4 29 db 13 db ci 2 14.33 db   28.33 db 20.33 db ci 2 24.33 db 5 28.67 db 23 db ci 2 14.33 db*   17.67 db 24.67 db ci 1 18.33 db 6 27.33 db 26 db ci 2 22 db*   18.33 db 27.67 db ci 1 22.33 db 7 19 db 20.33 db ci 1 23 db   9 db 26 db ci 1 17.67 db 8 21.33 db 9 db ci 2 9 db   20.33 db 25.67 db ci 1 20.33 db 9 28.5 db 23.67 db ci 2 23 db*   27.33 db 26.67 db ci 2 17 db* 10 19.67 db 19 db ci 2 14.33 db*   13 db 24 db ci 1 9 db* 11 27.33 db 25.67 db ci 2 26.5 db   29 db 26.33 db ci 2 23.67 db* mean 25.56 db 19.55 db 19.43 db 17.74 db   21.24 db 24.91 db 19.73 db 20.03 db * indicates bilateral benefit achieved (thus, perceive speech at lower snr compared with superior implant).   snr = signal-to-noise ratio; ci 1 = first cochlear implant; ci 2 = second cochlear implant; spin = speech-perception-in-noise ability; bici = both cochlear implants. table 4. snr values for ci 1, ci 2, superior implant and bici in spatially coincident speech and noise (n=11) participant spin with speech and noise presented from the front   ci 1 ci 2 superior ci bici 1 20 db 18 db ci 2 17.67 db* 2 23 db 22.33 db ci 2 22.67 db 3 18.67 db 9.67 db ci 2 12.33 db 4 23 db 20 db ci 2 20.67 db 5 22 db 22.67 db ci 1 18.33 db* 6 22.67 db 22 db ci 2 22.33 db 7 18.33 db 22.67 db ci 1 20.33 db 8 20.67 db 20 db ci 2 15 db* 9 22.33 db 21.67 db ci 2 22 db 10 18.33 db 16.33 db ci 2 12.33 db* 11 23 db 20.33 db ci 2 22.33 db average 21.06 db 19.61 db 19.15 db 18.37 db * indicates bilateral benefit achieved (thus, perceive speech at lower snr compared with superior implant). snr = signal-to-noise ratio; ci 1 = first cochlear implant; ci 2 = second cochlear implant; spin = speech-perception-in-noise ability; bici = both cochlear implants. figure 4 and table 3 demonstrate that with noise directed to ci 1 and ci 2, 64% (n=7/11) and 36% (n=4/11) of participants respectively demonstrated bilateral benefit during speech perception in spatially separated speech and noise. in contrast, with noise directed to ci 2 7 participants did not show a bilateral benefit but achieved a better snr value with their superior implant only (table 3). for 4 of these 7 participants (participants 1, 5, 6 and 7) their ci 1 remained superior in comparison to their performance with bilateral implant use. an average snr value of 19.43 db and 17.74 db was achieved respectively for the best performing ci and the bilateral listening condition with noise on ci 1. an average snr of 19.73 db and 20.03 db was achieved respectively for the best performing ci and the bilateral listening condition with noise on ci 2. thus, a bilateral benefit was present only when noise was directed to ci 1 and indicated an average improvement of 1.69 db. as displayed in figure 4, 36% of participants (n=4/11) (1, 5, 8 and 10) achieved bilateral benefit when speech and noise were spatially coincident. the observation point 9 in figure 4 emphasises the significantly better performance of participant 3 with his best performing implant, which was notably better than the other participants. a significant range of bilateral benefits in terms of snr in db was evident (15 22.67 db). an average snr of 19.15 db and 18.37 db was respectively achieved for the best performing ci and the bilateral listening condition. no statistically significant bilateral benefit (p>0.05) for speech perception in spatially separated speech and noise conditions (p=0.562 for noise on ci 1 and p=0.898 for noise on ci 2) or spatially coincident speech and noise (p=0.442) was found. fig. 4. speech-in-noise perception for the superior performing implant compared with the bilateral implant condition (n=11). a bilateral benefit is achieved when the participant perceives speech at a lower speech reception (sr) value (in db) with both implants (light bars) compared with the sr value (in db) of the superior performing implant (dark bars). box plots represent the median (thick horizontal line), lower and upper quartiles (ends of boxes), minimum and maximum values (ends of whiskers) and extreme values (dark circles). bilaleral spatial benefits the spatial benefits effects sizes are listed in table 5. the median head-shadow effect at 90° (0 db for ci 1 and ci 2) did not correspond significantly (p>0.05) with the ideal range of ≥3 db (schön et al., 2002; laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006). the median head-shadow effect at 180° for both ci 1 (4 db) and ci 2 (5 db) fell within the accepted range (≥3 db) on the 5% level of significance. the median squelch effect for both ci 1 and ci 2 (0 db and 1 db, respectively) was within the accepted range of ≤2 db (laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006; van deun et al., 2010) on a 5% level of significance. the median summation benefits for both ci 1 (0 db) and ci 2 (2 db) added to bici in the nf condition was within the accepted value range of ≤6 db (litovsky et al., 2006; wolfe et al., 2007; eapen, buss, adunka, pillsbury & buchman, 2009; van deun et al., 2010) at the 5% level of significance. the median srm benefit values were within the accepted range of 0 to 4 db (litovsky et al., 2006; van deun et al., 2010) only when the noise was directed to ci 1 at the 5% level of significance. an asymmetry between the values for noise directed to ci 1 versus ci 2 was observed, with greater values when the noise was presented to ci 1. in addition to the srm spatial benefit the improvement in speech perception in spatially distinct speech and noise from the addition of an ear with a better snr was calculated. this was calculated as the difference between the srt value for the left/right ear and the bici srt value in the nl/nr condition (van deun et al., 2010). the average value for adding the ear with the better snr to ci 1 (thus the contribution of ci 2) (9 db) is greater than the average value when the ear with the better snr is added to ci 2 (thus the contribution of ci 1) (5 db). the contribution of ci 2 therefore seems to be greater than the contribution of ci 1 for bilateral spatial benefit. table 5. bilateral spatial benefits effects sizes participant hs 90° ci 1 hs 90° ci 2 hs 180° ci 1 hs 180° ci 2 summation: with ci 1 added summation: with ci 2 added squelch: noise on ci 1 squelch: noise on ci 2 srm: noise on ci 1 srm: noise on ci 2 snr: better ear added to ci 1 snr: better ear added to ci 2 1 0 db 1 db 4 db 11 db 0 db 2 db -3 db 2 db 3 db -5 db 10 db 5 db 2 -6 db -6 db 1 db -1 db 0 db 0 db 0 db -1 db -5 db -6 db 1 db 0 db 3 -3 db 0 db 5 db 7 db -3 db 6 db 5 db 6 db 9 db -4 db 23 db 1 db 4 -5 db 7 db 1 db 7 db 0 db 2 db 4 db -1 db 6 db -4 db 15 db -4 db 5 4 db 0 db 11 db 2 db 4 db 4 db -1 db 7 db 2 db 0 db 12 db 6 db 6 4 db -4 db 9 db 2 db 0 db 0 db -4 db 4 db 0 db 0 db 5 db 5 db 7 9 db 2 db 10 db 6 db 2 db -2 db -9 db -3 db -3 db 3 db -4 db 8 db 8 0 db 11 db 1 db 17 db 5 db 6 db 0 db 0 db 6 db -5 db 12 db 5 db 9 -5 db -2 db 1 db 3 db 0 db 0 db 10 db 1 db -4 db 5 db 6 db 10 db 10 5 db -3 db 7 db 5 db 4 db 6 db 4 db 5 db -2 db 3 db 5 db 15 db 11 -6 db -5 db -2 db 1 db -2 db 1 db 5 db -1 db -4 db -1 db 1 db 3 db median 0 db  0 db  4 db*  5 db*  0 db* 2 db* 0 db* 1 db* 0 db*  -1 db 9 db 5 db * indicates effect sizes within the stated accepted value range. hs = head shadow; ci 1 = first cochlear implant; ci 2 = second cochlear implant; srm = spatial release from masking; snr = signal-to-noise ratio.   discussion speech-in-noise perception a bilateral benefit for speech-in-noise perception was found in the current study in the condition with noise directed to ci 1, yet it was not statistically significant. the majority of participants (64%; 7/11) demonstrated benefit with an average benefit of 1.69 db across the study sample. this improvement is significantly less than previously reported for simultaneously implanted adults, with an average bilateral benefit improvement of 5 db (cochlear corporation limited, 2005). the results are however comparable with previously reported bilateral benefit for sequentially implanted young adults (≤19 years of age) of 0.49 4.8 db in spatially separated speech and noise conditions (galvin et al., 2010). with noise directed to ci 1 and noise to ci 2, 64% and 36% of participants achieved a speech-in-noise-perception bilateral benefit, respectively. this is presumably because they were better able to process the speech signal when they listened with both their implants, as the spatial separation of the speech and noise sources better enabled them to segregate the speech signal from the noise (dunn, noble, tyler, kordus, gantz & haihong, 2010). the majority of participants (91% for ci 1; 55% for ci 2) demonstrated a performance advantage for the implant closest to the speech source compared with the implant closest to the noise source in unilateral listening conditions. this is known as a head-shadow benefit and was found to be a robust and significant benefit for speech-in-noise perception for dichotic listening conditions (listening condition where the speech and noise signals are spatially separated) in the vast majority of participants. this suggests some degree of bilateral benefit for speech-in-noise perception for the delayed sequentially implanted users in the current study (laszig et al., 2004). in the diotic listening condition (listening condition where the speech and noise signals are spatially coincident) no significant bilateral benefit for speech in noise was found among sequentially implanted adults. a bilateral benefit for speech-in-noise perception was observed in only 36% of participants (4/11) with the average benefit (0.78 db) less than the bilateral benefit of ≥1.4 db previously reported for simultaneously implanted users (tyler et al., 2002; cochlear corporation limited, 2005; ramsden et al., 2005; litvosky et al., 2006). most participants (9/11) demonstrated some bilateral speech-in-noise-perception benefit in at least one of the dichotic and/or diotic listening conditions. compared with previous studies of bilateral implantation in adults the current findings indicate that more robust improvements in speech-in-noise perception are typical of simultaneously implanted adult ci users than delayed sequentially implanted users (tyler et al., 2002; cochlear corporation limited, 2005; ramsden et al., 2005; litvosky et al., 2006). the concurrent stimulation of both peripheral and central auditory systems may lead to improved interaction between the ipsiand contralateral auditory pathways and provide more robust processing of signals (manrique et al., 2007). simultaneously implanted listeners required an snr of at least 6.8 db to achieve speech perception after 1 year, decreasing to 1.8 db after 4 years of bilateral simultaneous implantation (eapen et al., 2009). the latter snr is significantly lower than the snr values recorded for delayed sequentially implanted adults in this study (average snr: 18.83 db) despite most participants (6/11) having had bilateral experience of more than 4 years (table 2). the majority (9/11) of participants in the current study are delayed sequentially implanted users; the extent of bilateral benefits are limited further as a result of a lack of concurrent stimulation in auditory pathways compared with simultaneously and/or sequentially implanted adults (manrique et al., 2007). according to litovksy et al. (2006, 2009) a longer experience with bici use may be related to improvements in speech understanding in noise. a performance advantage is evident for 2 3 years after implantation after which a decline may be evident as users reach their performance plateau (manrique et al., 2007). since participants in the current study were evaluated at least 2 years post ci 2 they may already have reached their performance plateau phase, in contrast to benefits reported in previous studies that are typically measured between 3 and 9 months post implantation (cochlear corporation limited, 2005; galvin et al., 2010). bilateral spatial benefits the head-shadow effect at 180° was found to be the strongest and most robust bilateral spatial benefit for delayed sequentially implanted adults in this study. it was attained on at least 1 of the 2 unilateral ci comparisons for nearly all participants (9/11). furthermore, the median values of 4 db for the contribution from ci 1 and 5 db for the contribution from ci 2 to the head-shadow effect at 180° was well within the accepted range of 3 db except at 90° (laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006; van deun et al., 2010). results suggest that the greater the spatial separation between the speech signal and the noise source, the greater the head-shadow effect for the delayed sequentially implanted adults with bilateral benefits comparable with those reported in simultaneous and sequentially implanted adults(laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006; van deun et al., 2010). the squelch effect in general is small even in normal-hearing listeners where it is in the order of 3 db (eapen et al., 2009). it has been reported in only a few previous studies investigating bilateral benefit in adult ci users (laszig et al., 2004; eapen et al., 2009). current reports demonstrate a benefit of 2 db and even zero or negative effects in sequentially and simultaneously implanted adults (laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006; van deun et al., 2010). the majority of participants in the current study (55% for ci 1; 64% for ci 2) also presented with a squelch effect within the accepted benefit cut-off of ≤2 db which is comparable with those previously reported for sequentially and simultaneously implanted adults (laszig et al., 2004; litovsky et al., 2006; van deun et al., 2010). four participants obtained a negative squelch effect during the condition with noise on ci 1 (participants 1, 5, 6 and 7) and when the noise was directed to ci 2 (participants 2, 4, 7 and 11). when the noise was on ci 1, however, and this implant was added in the bilateral test, the range of negative values was greater (ranging from -1 db to -9 db) than the range of negative values for when the noise was directed to ci 2 (ranging from -1 db to -3 db). this might be explained by the fact that using the ci ipsilateral to the noise source resulted in degradation of the snr at that ear (van deun et al., 2010). the decrease in snr might be more significant when the noise is near the ci 1, which was the inferior functioning ear in the condition with noise directed to the ci 1 for participants 1, 5, and 6 (van deun et al., 2010). furthermore, the negative squelch effect values obtained by participants 1, 2, 4, 5, 6, 7 and 11 may also be attributed to distorted timing cues because of the lack of integration between the processing of the two processors (ramsden et al., 2005). all participants demonstrated a significant bilateral spatial benefit (p <0.05) for summation when ci 1 and ci 2 were both used to listen in diotic conditions. the results correspond with previous reports on bilateral sequential and simultaneous adult ci users that demonstrated a summation benefit of up to 6 db and/or no effect or negative effects (litovsky et al., 2006; wolfe et al., 2007; eapen et al., 2009; van deun et al., 2010). srm values of 0 db up to 4 db for sequentially and simultaneously implanted adults were reported in previous studies (litovsky et al., 2006; van deun et al., 2010). only in the noise directed to ci 1 condition did delayed sequentially implanted users’ median srm benefit (median: 0 db) correspond significantly (p <0.05) with the reported value range in simultaneously and sequentially implanted adults. an asymmetry was however observed with the srm[b] values indicating greater values when changing the noise to the ci 1. therefore, it may be assumed that the majority of participants performed better with their ci 2 relative to their ci 1 for speech-in-noise perception. the improvement in speech perception in spatially distinct speech and noise from adding the ear with a better snr was calculated in support of the srm. the average value for adding the snr better ear to ci 1, thus the contribution of ci 2, was greater than the average value when the snr better ear is added to ci 2 (thus the contribution of ci 1). therefore, this may further denote the superior contribution of ci 2 for bilateral benefit during speech perception in spatially separated speech and noise for the delayed sequentially implanted adults in this study. conclusion a bilateral benefit for speech-perception-in-noise abilities was noted in delayed sequentially implanted adults. however, statistically this was not significant. the delayed sequentially implanted users’ head-shadow effect at 180°, squelch effect, and summation effect for both ci 1 and ci 2 corresponded significantly (p<0.05) with previously reported cut-off normative values for bilateral spatial benefits in simultaneously and sequentially implanted adults. delayed sequentially implanted adults may achieve some bilateral benefit even after many years of unilateral implant use. yet, the extent of the bilateral benefit is less robust than reported for simultaneous and/or sequentially implanted adults (tyler et al., 2002; cochlear corporation limited, 2005; ramsden et al., 2005; litovsky et al., 2006). most participants (9/11) attained some bilateral speech-in-noise-perception benefit in at least one of the dichotic and/or diotic listening conditions. this underscores the importance of understanding the extent of these abilities, as they play an essential role in the rehabilitation of delayed sequential ci users. a key benefit of delayed sequential bilateral implantation appears to be related to the advantageous aspect of having hearing on both sides so that the ear with the more favourable environmental snr is always available. outcomes for these delayed sequentially implanted adults provide an indication of the bilateral benefit which may still be attainable after the official cut-off interval of more than 2 years for the ci 2 is exceeded.   references alpiner, j. g., & mccarthy, p. a. (2000). rehabilitative audiology: children and adults. 3rd ed. philadelphia: lippincott williams & wilkins. balkany, t., hodges, a., telischi, f., hoffman, r., madell, j., parisier, s., gantz, b., tyler, r., & peters, r. (2008). william house cochlear implant study group: position statement on bilateral cochlear implantation. otology & neurotology, 29(2), 107-108. cochlear corporation limited. candidacy criteria for children and adults (n.d.). retrieved on 26 february 2008 from http://www.cochlear.com. cochlear corporation limited. (2005). outcomes using bilateral cochlear implants in adults. [brochure]. australia. retrieved on 12 november 2012 from: http://sites.cochlear.com/binaural/sites/default/files/outcomes_using_bilateral.pdf dunn, c. c., noble, w., tyler, r. s., kordus, m., gantz, b. j., & haihong, j. (2010). bilateral and unilateral cochlear implant users compared on speech perception in noise. ear and hearing, 31(2), 296-298. doi:10.1097/aud.0b013e3181c12383 eapen, r. j., buss, e., adunka, m. c., pillsbury iii, h. c., & buchman, c. a. (2009). hearing-in-noise benefits after bilateral simultaneous cochlear implantation continue to improve 4 years after implantation. otology & neurotology, 30, 153-159. doi:10.1097/mao.0b013e3181925025 field, a. (2005). discovering statistics using spss. london: sage publications. galvin, k. l., hughes, k. c., & mok, m. (2010). can adolescents and young adults with prelingual hearing loss benefit from a second, sequential cochlear implant? international journal of audiology, 49(5), 368-377. doi:10.3109/14992020903470767 gantz, b. j., tyler, r. s., rubenstein, j. t. wolaver a., lowder m., abbas, p., brown, c., hughes, m. & preece, j. p. (2002). binaural cochlear implants placed during the same operation. otology & neurotology, 23,169-180. doi:10.1097/00129492-200203000-00012 harrel, r. w. (2002). puretone evaluation. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology. (5th ed., pp. 71-87). philadelphia: lippincott williams & wilkins. hughes, m. l., van der werf, k. r., brown, c. j., abbas, p. j., kelsay, d. m. r., teagle, h. f. b., & lowder, m. w. (2001). longitudinal study of electrode impedance, electrically evoked compound action potential and behavioral measures in nucleus 24 cochlear implant users. ear and hearing, 22(6): 471-486. doi:10.1097/00003446-200112000-00004 laszig, r., aschendorff, a., stecker, m., müller-deile j, maune s, dillier n, weber b, hey m, begall k, lenarz t, battmer rd, böhm m, steffens t, strutz j, linder t, probst r, allum j, westhofen m, doering w. (2004). benefits of bilateral electrical stimulation with the nucleus cochlear implant in adults: 6-month postoperative results. otology & neurotology, 25 (6), 958-968. doi:10.1097/00129492-200411000-00016 leedy, p. d., & ormrod. j. e. (2005). practical research: planning and design. 8th ed. princeton, n.c.: merill publishing company. litovsky, r. y., parkinson, a., arcaroli, j., & arcaroli, j. (2009). spatial hearing and speech intelligibility in bilateral cochlear implant users. ear and hearing, 30(4), 419-431. doi:10.1097/aud.0b013e3181a165be litovsky, r. y., parkinson, a., arcaroli, j., peters, r., lake, j., johnstone, p., & yu, g. (2004). bilateral cochlear implants in adults and children. archives of otolaryngology head and neck surgery, 130(5), 648-655. doi:10.1001/archotol.130.5.648 litovsky, r. y., parkinson, a., arcaroli, j., & sammeth, c. (2006). simultaneous bilateral cochlear implantation in adults: a multicenter clinical study. ear and hearing, 27(6), 714-731. doi:10.1097/01.aud.0000246816.50820.42 lustig, r. l., & wackym, p. a. (2005). bilateral cochlear implantation. operative techinques in otolaryngoloy head and neck surgery, 16(2), 125-130. doi:10.1016/j.otot.2005.04.003 manrique, m., huarte, a., valdivieso, a., & pérez, b. (2007). bilateral sequential implantation in children. audiological medicine, 5(4), 224-231. doi:10.1080/16513860701705173 miller, i., & miller, m. (2004). john e. freund’s mathematical statistics with applications. london: pearson prentice hall. moore, j. a., & teagle, h. f. b. (2002). an introduction to cochlear implant technology, activation, and programming. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 33, 156-161. doi:10.1044/0161-1461(2002/013) muller, a. m. u., & de stadler, l. (1987). cid sinne afrikaanse vertaling. unpublished. university of stellenbosch, cape town, south africa. neuman, a. c., haravon, a., sislian, n., & waltzman, s. b. (2007). sound-direction identification with bilateral cochlear implants. ear and hearing, 28(1), 73-82. doi:10.1097/01.aud.0000249910.80803.b9 peters, b. j., litovsky, r., parkinson, a., & lake, j. (2007). importance of age and postimplantation experience on speech perception measures in children with sequential bilateral cochlear implants. otology & neurotology, 28, 649-657. doi:10.1097/01.mao.0000281807.89938.60 ramsden, r., greenham, p., o’driscoll, m., mawman, d., proops, d., craddock, l., et al. (2005). evaluation of bilaterally implanted adult subjects with the nucleus 24 cochlear implant system. otology & neurotology, 25(6), 988-998. doi:10.1097/01.mao.0000185075.58199.22 schön, f., müller, j., & helms, j. (2002). speech reception thresholds obtained in a symmetrical four-loudspeaker arrangement from bilateral users of med-el cochlear implants. otology & neurotology, 23, 710-714. doi:10.1097/00129492-200209000-00018 steyn, a. g. w., smit, c. f., du toit, s. h. c., & strasheim, c. (2003). modern statistics in practice. pretoria: van schaik. teoh, s. w., pisoni, d. b., & miyamoto, r. t. (2004). cochlear implantation in adults with prelingual deafness. part 1. clinical results. the laryngoscope, 114(9), 1536-1540. doi:10.1097/00005537-200409000-00006 tyler, r. s., dunn, c. c., witt, s. a., & noble, w. g. (2007). speech perception and localization with adults with bilateral sequential cochlear implants. ear and hearing, 28(2) supplement, 86 90. doi:10.1097/aud.0b013e31803153e2 tyler, r. s., gantz, b. j., rubinstein, j. t. wilson b. s., parkinson a. j., wolaver a., preece j. p., witt s., lowder m. w. (2002). three-month results with bilateral cochlear implants. ear and hearing, 23(1) supplement, 80-89. doi:10.1097/00003446-200202001-00010 van deun, l., van wieringen, a., & wouters j. (2010). spatial speech perception benefits in young children with normal hearing and cochlear implants. ear and hearing, 31(5), 702-713. wilber, l. a. (2002). calibration: puretone, speech and noise signals. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology (5th ed., pp. 50-67). philadelphia: lippincott williams & wilkins. wolfe, j., baker, s., caraway, t., kasulis h., mears a., smith j., swim l., wood m. (2007). 1-year postactivation results for sequentially implanted bilateral cochlear implant users. otology & neurotology, 28(5), 589-596. doi:10.1097/mao.0b013e318067bd24 wolfe, j., & schafer, e. c. (2010). effects of accessory-mixing ration on performance with personal fm and cochlear implants. in: c. deconde johnson, d. e. lewis, h. e. mülder & l. m. thibodeau (eds.), achieving clear communication employing sound solutions 2008: proceedings of the first international virtual conference on fm (pp. 146-153). stäfa, switzerland: phonak ag. 63 communication intervention in an adolescent with profound cognitive impairment and autistic features ilze pansegrouw and erna alant department of communication pathology university of pretoria abstract the service delivery model currently used with a large proportion of profoundly cognitively impaired (pci) persons, results in the under-utilization of their potential and often contributes to social isolation. by providing communication and independence training the self-actualisation potential and the right to power and control, is recognised. this single case study describes the implementation of a communication intervention model with a pci adolescent. his mother was trained in the use of picture symbol task analysis as well as positive reinforcement to promote change in the adolescent's communication skills and independence. results indicated significant changes in the skills of both participants and highlighted the mother's need for support to meet the demands of change. opsomming diekenmerke van die diensleweringsmodel by 'n groot persentasie kognitief erg gestremde (keg-) persone, resulteer dikwels in die onderbenutting van hul potensiaal en vir sommige in sosiale isolasie. deur die voorsiening van kommunikasieintervensie en onafhanklikheidsopleiding word in die selfverwesenlikingspotensiaal van die keg-persoon voorsien en word sy/haar selfhandhawingsregte erken. hierdie enkelgevalstudie beskryf'n kommunikasie-intervensiemodel wat by 'n kegadolessent toegepas is. die moeder van die adolessent is in die gebruik van prentsimbooltaakanalises en positiewe versterking opgelei met die oog op verandering by die kommunikasievaardighede en onafhanklikheid van die adolessent. resultate het betekenisvolle verandering in die doelstellings van die studie aangetoon en die ondersteuningsbehoeftes van die moeder ten opsigte van die eise wat verandering meebring, is beklemtoon. key words: profound cognitive impairment (pci), communication intervention, independence training. introduction due to limited provision of services and facilities the needs of a significant proportion of cognitively impaired persons (cip) are not being met (alant & emmett, 1995). services such as speech therapy are often not accessible due to beliefs that poor progress in therapy indicates that these persons do not benefit'from such therapy (calculator, 1988a). consequently, a large percentage of cip persons reside in segregated "special-care" units where total physical care is the only form of service provided (lea, 1990). in addition, parents of those who reside at home report a lack of formal an informal societal assistance such as respite care to relieve caregiver stress (singer & irvin, 1991). the results of limited services to both parents and cip persons are far-reaching. it is known that the clinical picture of a large proportion of these persons relates to the nature of service delivery and the intervention approach (lea, 1990). the intervention approach to profound cognitive impaired (pci) persons (i.q. 20 and less) is clearly demonstrated in the categorisation of these persons as "untrainable" (grover, 1990). the term "untrainable" implies that the person does riot have the ability to benefit from education or training and is therefore dependent on total physical care (koordineringskomitee: jaar van gestremde persone, 1987). these persons represent approximately l%-2% of the cognitively handicapped population (subcommittee on mental retardation, 1987a). clinical features include the description of a high percentage of self injurious and socially unacceptable behaviour, as well as low motivation relating to the development of independence skills (sailor & guess, 1983). language and communication impairment represent the most pervasive problem of the majority of pci persons and have been linked to the high incidence of severe behavioural problems in this group of people (durandt, 1990; yoder & villarruel, 1988). due to frequent impairment in motor abilities, functional speech skills are often severely limited (sailor & guess, 1983). apart from the individual's inability to communicate, however, the inability to respond to the interaction attempts of primary caregivers often results in the gradual reduction of primary caregiver attempts to interact with the impaired person. this leads to low caregiver expectations regarding communication from the impaired person, reduced opportunities to support the development of language, communication and social skills and results in severe difficulty die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 ilze pansegrouw and erna alant in forming or participating in even the most basic social relationships (kirchener, 1991). primary caregivers often compensate and adapt to the impaired person in a way that contributes to the development of "learned helplessness" (yoder & villarruel, 1988). learned helplessness is seen in the anticipation of all needs of the impaired person and results in low demands for communication and low participation in domestic activities and household routines. these features result in reduced social environmental integration of the impaired person. one of the most important indicators of the quality of life, is the extent of a person's social integration (haring, 1991). from the above discussion the following principles of communication intervention can be formulated. these principles provide important criteria for successful intervention with pci persons: quality of life is determined by the extent to which the person is socially integrated in his daily environment. independence skills provide opportunities for active participation in daily activities and routines within the environment (haring, 1991). the quantity and quality of communication interaction between the pci person and the environment, relate closely to the communication skills of the impaired person. the pci person cannot be viewed apart from his/ her daily environment; this person is viewed as one component of the environment in which he lives and spends most of his time (gottlieb, 1988). the above implies that the term "client" comprises both the impaired person and his environment. in the following, the application of the aforementioned principles to develop a model for communication intervention with a pci person, is discussed. proposed model for intervention in this study a communication intervention model was developed to address the principles and important criteria i priority skills ψ social sub-skills independence sub-skills i i communication skills * a.d.l. all family/other environments caregivers i 4 daily environment primary caregiver i i priorities priorities and skill i i predictability psychological and security sub-skills context and responsiveness i sub-skills i i homeostasis social isolation and stress action and discourse skills related to communication and training ν programplanning key: * a.d.l. = activities of daily life * pci person = profound cognitive impaired person figure 1: client approach: communication intervention model the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) £ o i n i n u n i c a t i o n intervention in an adolescent with autistic features for intervention with pci persons. this model is presented in figure 1. figure 1 provides criteria to identify the daily environment and the primary caregiver as well as priority skills that were identified for the client. central to this model is the concept of quality of life, thus the facilitation of social interaction and the developm e n t of independence for active participation within this c o n t e x t . t h e functioning of the person is the product of int e r a c t i o n between the person and the environment ( e h r e n & lenz, 1989). figure 1 demonstrates a clear distinction between the family or other caregivers and the primary c a r e g i v e r as well as between the daily environment (where the person spends most of his time) and other environments w h i c h he might visit on occasion. the primary caregiver is identified as the person with whom the pci person has a high frequency of contact, who elicits a high frequency of communication initiations and responses from the person and with whom there exists a strong affective bond (owens & rogerson, 1988). for obvious reasons, this would be the most important starting point in intervention. figure 1 demonstrates that for the pci person, social integration, i.e., the acquisition of communication skills and becoming independent within this context, is a high priority. the extent of the person's ability to develop stable social relationships in order to achieve active social integration within the daily environment, provides and indication of the needs for support of the person. the development and maintenance of active, social integration is dependent on functional communication skills which eire prerequisites for the development and maintenance of social integration (rustin & ruhr, 1989). it follows that pci persons, who often do not develop functioned speech production skills and who do not have access to aac (augmentative and alternative communication) methods, will experience severe difficulty in forming and maintaining social relationships with others in the environment if support in the form of communication intervention is not provided. similarly, skills related to activities of daily living (activities occurring frequently and that eire essential for the maintenance of hygiene arid order within the daily environment) determine the level of independence within the daily environment (eshilian, haney & falvey, 1989). homeostasis (see figure 1) within the daily environment refers to environmental attempts which aim at creating predictability and security to maintain the existing structure and order of the environment (lund, 1986). radical change of environmental homeostasis (such as change as a result of intervention) poses a threat to environmental predictability and security and if not managed sensitively, can result in resistance to change (conti-ramsden, 1985). action and discourse routines on the other hand, refer to frequently occurring, highly predictable action or conversation events that, once they begin, unfold in the same manner each time they occur (lund, 1986). action routines thus refer to an event such as ball play where the action forms the main activity between the persons involved while discourse routines refer to an event such as greeting where the conversation forms the main activity between the persons involved. routines play an important role in the maintenance of homeostasis and are equally important to the development of social and communication skills (rowland & stremel-campbell, 1987). when looking at the primary caregiver, it can be seen in figure 1 that the psychological context, as well as skills profound cognitive impairment and 65 n r i n i ? t ° . c a ; i ! g i v e : responsiveness training are deemed priorities in this intervention model. socially constructed factors and processes are often the cause for the primary caregiver s social isolation and stress (lea, 1990) social isolation and associated stressors are found in the presence of stigmatisation and limited support services (lea 1990; helm & kozloff, 1986). limited knowledge and skill can, amongst others, result in counter productive interaction patterns such as limited primary caregiver responsiveness (calculator, 1988b). primary caregiver responsiveness is acknowledged as a key factor in the development of interaction and communication for the pci person (girolametto, greenberg & manolson, 1986). in considering responsiveness, the skills of the primary caregiver in terms of how much communication is expected of the pci person and whether utterances directed at the person are interaction and discourse orientated, are taken into account (siegel-causey & downing, 1987). it is against this background that a study was conducted to explore the applicability of the above model of communication intervention for a pci person adolescent with autistic features. specific priority skills and other skill areas were identified according to the model described in figure 1. methodology aims of the study the main aim of the study was to develop a communication intervention programme for a pci person by means of identified priority skills. the sub-aims were three-fold: firstly a pilot study was conducted to identify the needs of the subject and his mother relating to priority skills, to conduct an intervention trial and to develop relevant intervention materials. the second sub-aim was to implement the programme that was developed by describing the communication of the mother and her skills at training the subject and to describe the communication and independence at performing action routines, of the subject. the third sub-aim was to describe the results of identified priority skills of the subject and his mother during the six week intervention period as well as the effect of the withdrawal of intervention during week six. research design a quasi-experimental single case study with a multiple baseline (aba) design with withdrawal was selected for use in this study. the multiple baseline design for the measurement of change in behaviour patterns is well suited to execute aba comparisons (kearns, 1986). the mother-subject dyad was used whereby the behaviours of both were recorded. the research design is presented in table 1. it is known that motivation plays a significant role in the learning process and that repeated failure most often results in a decrease in motivation as well as task avoidance (haney & falvey, 1989). the "feed the cat" routine was completely unfamiliar to the subject. because both the "make the bed" and "set the table" routines were not completely unfamiliar to the subject it was decided to introduce these before the "feed the cat" routine to make sure that success was highly likely with the first two routines. a withdrawal period could establish whether changes in the subject and the mother could be attributed to the intervention. the action routines which were introduced one after suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 ilze pansegrouw and erna alant the other, were performed for the whole intervention period (not during withdrawal) because it is known that pci persons forget learning material which is not reinforced regularly and on a daily basis (brown, 1983). subject for the study, a fifteen (15) year old pci subject was selected from nine possible candidates at a school for autistic children in pretoria. the subject received individual speech therapy aimed at the development of speech production for the first six years in school. the production and imitation of a few single words are reported between the ages of two and four with a gradual decrease in vocalisations since then. the subject was introduced to aac at the age of eleven years. during the initial training stages, photographs of highly motivational objects and actions for the subject were used and after six months, makaton picture symbols (1985), were introduced. table 2 provides the psychiatric diagnosis of the subject, formal assessment results as well as a description of the communication and independence skills of the subject. mother the mother, who is the primary caregiver, took very good care of the subject. she has an honours degree in biochemistry but was not employed at the time of the study. other family members included the father and an older non-impaired brother. pilot study the pilot study consisted of two phases. phase one aimed at needs identification and the gathering of information on the pre-intervention state of priority skills of the subject and his mother. the information gathered during phase one, was utilised to conduct phase two of the pilot study. phase two was aimed at an intervention trial with the class teacher of the subject as well as the development of an instrument to measure change during intervention. phase 1: needs identified by the mother during phase one the mother expressed the view that increased independence in the area of a.d.l. was the subject's main need and that she would like to see the subject become independent in setting the table at home (an action table 1: research design routine which he has been exposed to at school), making his bed (inconsistent attempts at this had been made previously by mother) and feed the cat (to which he had no previous exposure). the mother expressed limited knowledge and self-confidence in training the subject. during video-recordings of two domestic action routines (loading the washing machine and unpacking the dishwasher), the subject indicated a strong need for positive reinforcement responses from his mother when he succeeded in certain parts of action routines. significant features of the mother's communication included the introduction and repetition of a limited amount of conversational topics with the subject, a high percentage of commands and low percentage of responsive utterances/actions. it was noted that due to the under-utilisation by the subject of his aac system (refer to discussion of pci subject), his communication intentions were often not understood by his mother. at the end of phase one, two priority skills, namely communication skills and skills at training the subject, were selected for the mother. for the subject, communication skills and independence at performing those action routines that were selected by his mother (setting the table, making the bed and feeding the cat), were identified. phase 2: development of action routines and preliminary testing during phase two a concrete positive reinforcement sticker chart (commercially available white, square, peeloff stickers) as well as three preliminary picture symbol task analysis charts were developed for each one of the three action routines that were identified by the mother. a task analysis is the "breaking down" (analysis) of a learning task into smaller, more manageable steps with the aim of introducing the task as a "chain" of related steps. this task analysis method is an acknowledged strategy in domestic independence training of cognitively impaired persons (sailor & guess, 1983). although three preliminary picture symbol task analysis charts were developed, only "set the table" was used in the intervention trial. the subject's teacher was asked to participate as "primary caregiver" in the trial with the aim of testing the usefulness and appropriateness of intervention materials. the teacher was trained to use the task analysis and positive reinforcement chart and the trial performance of "set the table" action routine was video recorded. for the purpose of this study only the primary caregiver week 0 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 6 13/7 13/7-20/7 20/7-27/7 27/7-3/8 3/8-10/8 10/8-17/8 17/8-22/8 22/8 set the table a bl bl bl bl a l a l β β β make the bed a bl bl bl bl a l a l β β β feed the cat a bl bl bl a l / a l β β key: a = baseline measurements β = measurements β1 = performance of action routines a l = withdrawal the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) u n i c a t i o n intervention in an adolescent with profound cognitive impairment and com"1 1 autistic features f r to discussion on proposed model for intervention) of tte subject was selected and the class teacher was only involved in the intervention trial. measurement instrument the measurement instrument was developed to record and describe information on the identified priority skills of the subject and his mother. (see introductory paragraphs 67 for the discussion and relevance of the selected priority skills). skills selected for measurement were identified by analysing each priority skill into sub-skills. the selected behaviours for observation were supplemented by observing video-recordings of week 0. sub-skills were either marked + (positive) or (negative). positive behaviours (+) were those where an increase indicated a positive change and negative (-) behaviours were those where an increase indicated a negative change. table 2: subject: psychiatric diagnosis, formal assessment results, communication and independence skills results date description 1. psychiatric diagnosis: 1979 (*c.a. 2 years) 2. formal assessment results: griffiths developmental scales: # speech and hearing: # personal/social: # mental age: # general quotient: reynell language developmental scales: # receptive: # expressive: peabody picture vocabulary test: 3. communication and independence communication and interaction: * requests: * protest: (*c.a. 15 years) 1992 1992 1992 1992 : use of picture symbols: independence: * action routines: selfcare: "profound mental retardation in a brain damaged child with a few autistic features such as spinning of objects, stereotyped, repetitive movements and handling of objects as well as poor interpersonal contact and relationships" 14 months 35 months 25 months 14 1 year 11 months below 1 year 2 years 1 month -the subject indicated a need for communication and interaction with adults but no such need with peers. during this time an increase in the use of picture symbols to request objects at home and with selected adults was reported. * he pulled people by the hand towards required object, brought object to adult or established eye contact + noises + pointing at object * pushed people and objects away or ignored requests, commands and communication of the partner * he used picture symbols only for request purposes when the aforementioned request strategies were not successful or when he was reminded to use his symbols. his picture symbol system comprised of ten symbols which were a combination of makaton (walker, 1985) and picsyms (carlson, 1985) picture symbols. he used one makaton sign (walker, 1972) namely "thank you/ please". this sign was used spontaneously to request permission (often when he was refused a request) and was also used to indicate confusion * he enjoyed domestic action routines such as unpacking the dishwasher or loading the washing machine and spontaneously participated. adult assistance was, however, required * although he was mainly independent regarding selfcare, he needed assistance with dressing, bath and tooth brushing key: * c.a. = chronological age d i e suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 appendix 1 provides the grouping of sub-skills of priority skills of the subject and his mother. appendix 2 provides the grid which was used to record and analyse data regarding sub-skills of the mother's skills at training the subject. the sub-skills of the other three priority skills, i.e. sub-skills of the mother's communication skills and the subskills of the independence and communication skills of the subject, were recorded onto three other grids. ilze pansegrouw and erna alant results of preliminary test results of phase two not only indicated the effectiveness of procedures but also highlighted the need for minor changes. a"theme symbol" was added as well as a separate column on the task analysis chart for positive reinforcement stickers. the mother suggested the use of "spot", "donald duck" and "ladybug" commercially available sticktable 3: picture symbol task analysis of action routines action routines size of task analysis chart sub-components of task analysis and picture symbol systems used size of symbols colour of carton to which picture symbols are attached size of coloured carton background placement of symbols on task analysis chart * other symbols in * other symbols in sequence: sequence: 1. plate (m) 1. pillow off (s) 2. placemats (s) 2. duvet off (s) 3. knife (m) 3. smooth (s) 4. fork (m) 4. pillow on (s) 5. glass (m) 5. duvet on (s) 6. serviette (s) set the table 34 χ 64 cm * theme symbol: set the table (s) 9 x 8 cm yellow 10 χ 9 cm theme symbol set the table 1. 2. 3. 4. 5. 6. make the bed 34 χ 42.2 cm * theme symbol: make the bed (s) 9 x 8 cm pink 10 χ 9 cm theme symbol make the bed 1. 2. 5. feed the cat 34 χ 64 cm * theme symbol: feed the cat (s) * other symbols in sequence: 1. bring the milk-bowl (m&s) 2. bring the food-bowl (s) 3. bring the milk (m) 4. fork (m) 5. bring the cat food (s) plus wrapper of cat food tin 6. fill with milk (s) 7. food in bowl (s) 8& 9. put down (s) 9 x 8 cm white 10 χ 9 cm theme-symbol feed the cat 2. 3. 4. 6. 8. 9. * each picture symbol was covered with desifix and attached to the chart with prestik. prestik was used so that the mother could, when necessary, change the sequence of the symbol arrangement on the chart placement inside of the kitchen cupboard door where the plates and the glasses are stored inside of the subject's clothes cupboard in his room inside o f kitchen cupboard door where the cat food is stored key: (m) = makaton (s) = self-developed symbols the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u n i c a t i o n intervention in an adolescent with autistic features e r s because the subject was familiar with these icons from watching animated videos and indicated enjoyment whenever he saw them. the mother of the subject was consulted regarding the size and placement inside the family home of the task analysis charts. these suggestions were combined with the results of the trial intervention to determine the features and application of training materials used in the main study. the main study training materials l&ble 3 provides information on the picture symbol task analysis charts of the three action routines and table 4 provides the features and placement of positive reinforcement stickers that were used as training material for the adolescent. more detail on the development of these materials can be obtained in pansegrouw (1994). the implementation of action routines were video recorded in the family's home during time slots when these activities would normally take place. "set the table" action routines were recorded at 19:00, "make the bed" at 07:00 and "feed the cat" at 10:30. profound cognitive impairment and 69 measurement recordings (b-phase; see table 1) were done at the same time in which these action routines would normally take place in the family's home. the first measurement video recording was made of the "set the table" routine seven days after the introduction thereof. on the same day the task analysis chart, instructions for use and training for the "make the bed" action routine was introduced to the mother. she received instructions to continue the "set the table" action routine during the following week. seven days after the "make the bed" action routine was introduced, this action routine was measured for the first time and the "set the table" routine measured for the second time. on the same day the "feed the cat" action routine was introduced to the mother. she received instructions to continue with the "set the table" and "make the bed" action routines in the following week. after seven days, the "feed the cat" action routine was measured for the first time and the "make the bed" routine for the second time. the mother received instructions to continue with the performing of the three action routines for another seven days and then to withdraw intervention for two weeks. on the last day of the two week withdrawal period, the third measurements of the "set the table" and "make the bed" as well as the second measurement of "feed the cat" routine were done. procedures data collection baseline video recordings (α-phase; see table 1) of the three action routines were done at the family home on the first day of the experiment (week 0). the mother was asked to introduce the subject to the three selected action routines one after the other. for the baseline recordings the mother received no training or task analysis charts. after the baseline recordings, on the same day, the mother received the task analysis chart, instructions for use and relevant training for the "set the table" action routine. ί training procedures and instructions training procedures included detailed instructions to the mother on how to perform and train action routines once a day. typed, as well as verbal instructions, specified utterances to be used an non-verbal guidance (such as pointing, gesture, physical guidance) to attract, direct and maintain the attention of the adolescent during the action routine. instructions to pause regularly in order to create opportunity for the subject to initiate communication and to demonstrate independence development, were given to the mother. she was also instructed to respond to all communication attempts and other noise/sounds that the subject table 4 : concrete positive reinforcement action routine 1 set the table make the bed feed the cat i sticker j spot (otto) donald duck lady bug colour of carton background yellow pink white size of stickers 2,5 χ 2,5 cm 2,5 χ 2,5 cm 2,5 χ 2,5 cm placement vertically in 4 cm column on the right side of the chart vertically in 4 cm column on the right side of the chart vertically in 4 cm column on the right side of the chart placement on task analysis chart 1 placement on task analysis chart 1 * theme symbol ~ picture symbol # positive reinforcement stickers 1 # 1 1 placement on task analysis chart 1 * theme symbol ~ picture symbol # positive reinforcement stickers placement on task analysis chart 1 * theme symbol ~ picture symbol # positive reinforcement stickers ~l 1 # 1 1 placement on task analysis chart 1 * theme symbol ~ picture symbol # positive reinforcement stickers placement on task analysis chart 1 * theme symbol ~ picture symbol # positive reinforcement stickers ~l 1 # 1 placement on task analysis chart 1 * theme symbol ~ picture symbol # positive reinforcement stickers placement on task analysis chart 1 * theme symbol ~ picture symbol # positive reinforcement stickers ~ ι ι ~i * 1 1 placement on task analysis chart 1 * theme symbol ~ picture symbol # positive reinforcement stickers die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 produced during the performance of action routines. instructions for the performance of the "set the table" action routine were as follows: mother was asked to place four each of plates, knives, forks, glasses, placemats and serviettes alongside each other in the cupboard where these objects are kept. she was asked to show the subject where the objects were placed. mother said "s. it is time now to set the table; we are going to eat." mother directed/physically cued subject to the task analysis chart. mother pointed to the theme symbol and said: "look, it is time now to set the table" (she directed the subject's attention to the theme symbol.) mother looked at the subject, pointed to the theme symbol and said, "s. please get the plates from the cupboard." mother directed the subject's attention to the plate picture symbol and pointed to the cupboard where the plates were kept. if he understood the instruction and performed the action without any trouble, mother said: "put a plate on the table for dad, put a plate down for α., a plate for me and a plate for yourself." mother was encouraged to compliment verbal instructions with gesture and facial expression. should the subject experience difficulty with any instruction, mother repeated the instruction and physically cued him through the action. after all the plates were placed on the table, the other utensils (forks, knives, placemats, glasses and serviettes) were placed on the table by using the same procedures and instructions. instructions for the use of positive reinforcement were as follows: after the performance of a correct action by the subject the mother was requested to do or say the following: "very good!" (positive reinforcement: verbal); smile (positive reinforcement: smile); squeeze his hand/shoulder (positive reinforcement: physical) and to place a sticker (positive reinforcement: concrete) in the left upper corner of the appropriate picture symbol. after the attachment of the sticker, mother pointed to the sticker and repeated: 'very good!" data analysis and statistical processing verbatim transcriptions of video material of the interaction between the mother and subject during action routines were done. due to the fact that the subject did not communicate verbally, his communication was described by recording occurrences of pointing, smiles, gesture, eye contact as well as communicative sounds and noises on the relevant recording sheets. (appendix 2 provides an example of a recording grid). recording was done by counting each occurrence of a sub-skill on the transcriptions. tallies were recorded onto the four different grids (see appendix 2). the friedman variance analysis (hollander & wolfe, 1973) was applied to the data to indicate p-valilze pansegrouw and erna alant ues (significance of change) of the sub-skills of the priority skills over the six week intervention period. the p. value indicates significance of change of the difference between week 0 and any other measurement between week 0 and week 6. raw scores were processed to tallies p e r minute and the average percentage change in relation to week 0 (baseline), calculated. the percentage change refers to the percentage change in relation to week 0. for equivalence of action routines, interpolation· was done to determine percentage change for week three of "set the table". in this way the change in priority skills for week 2 for both "set the table" and "make the bed" as well as week 3 and 6 (in relation to week 0) for all action routines, could be presented. at the comparison of the sub-skills for all action routines only the percentage change at weeks 3 and 6 are presented. this was done because the "feed the cat" action routine was only introduced in week 2 and measured one week later (see table 1). through these procedures, changes in sub-skills of priority skills at weeks 0 , 3 and 6 could be calculated for all action routines. results and discussion the results of intervention, i.e. a description of identified priority skills of the subject and his mother during the six week intervention period as well as the effect of withdrawal of intervention during week six, follows. figure 2 represents the overall results of the percentage change of the two priority skills of the mother (communication skills and skills at training the subject) and the two priority skills of the subject (communication skills and independence at performing action routines) in weeks three and six. from figure 2 it is evident that positive changes were seen in the results of the priority skills of the subjects. the subject benefitted from and developed new skills during the six week intervention period. at the time of the study, the chronological age of the subject was fifteen. he then was found to have a m.a. (mental age) of 25 months 4 0 0 3 5 0 3 0 0 2 5 0 2 0 0 1 5 0 100 5 0 0 % change 0 0 | 0 0 week 0 4 7 53 1 w3(tbc) w6(tbc) m/c ih s / c ees m/s μ s/i key: (tbc) = set the table; β = make the bed; c + weed the cat m/c = mother: communication skills s/c = subject: communication skills / m/s = mother: skills at training the subject s/i = subject: independence at performing action routines y figure 2: change of priority skills at weeks three and six x the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) £ , o n u n u n i c a t i o n intervention in an adolescent with autistic features and a g.q. (general quotient) of 14. according to existing y c h o m e t r i c categorisation the subject would be " u n t r a i n a b l e " which implies an inability to benefit from education/training and consequently being dependent upon physical care (grover, 1990; koordineringskomitee: jaar van g e s t r e m d e persone, 1987). results of this study support the view that clinical features are dynamic and often r e f l e c t the extent and nature of the service delivery model (lea, 1990). physical care as the only form of service delivery to the subject, would most certainly have resulted in the under-utilisation of potential. the subject's communication and independence skills when looking at figures 2, 3 and 4 a significant difference in results between weeks nil to three (during intervention) and weeks nil to six (after intervention withdrawal) in the priority skills of the subject, is seen. results in figure 3 indicate that in weeks nil to six independence skills either increased or remained nearly the same. from the same figure it can also be seen that communication skills (apart from picture symbol communication which he used to indicate that he wanted to terminate routines) decreased from weeks nil to six. from these results it seems that the subject did not lose/forget skills during intervention withdrawal but instead presented poor motivation and unwillingness to co-operate. it is known that regular, consistent practice of skills with a familiar person in the environment, as well as the provision of external structure to facilitate productive functioning are important requirements for intervention success with pci persons (siegel-causey & downing, 1987). during the withdrawal period provision of external structure and regular performance of action routines decreased significantly. for this reason it becomes clear that so called generalisation problems often reflect a situation where behaviour has generalised but where reinforcement profound cognitive impairment and 71 key: p/c = picture symbol communication; r/r/c = requests for recognition/confirmation; s/r = smiles for recognition; i/c = intentional communication; c/w/ a/ = performs commands without assistance from mother; sc/i = performs sub-components of action routines independent/without command from mother; a/c/pr = anticipates concrete positive reinforcement figure 3: subject: selected sub-skills of communication skills and independence (all action routines) for the regular use of the behaviour, was insufficient (johnson & koegel, 1982). increases were seen in the results of the subject's requests for and smiles for recognition, as well as anticipation of concrete positive reinforcement. these results indicate that the subject's related needs were addressed by providing his mother with positive reinforcement training. it also suggests that the often described clinical feature of poor inner motivation of the pci person to develop skills that would decrease dependence of persons in the environment, could be changed by providing motivation (positive reinforcement) as the first step in the learning process (sailor & guess, 1983; haney & falvey, 1989). when looking at increases in the results of the communication skills of the subject (figures 2, 3 and 4), it seems that the provision of picture symbol task analysis charts to introduce action routines, provided the context in which communication between the mother and the subject was encouraged through the provision of common, specific themes that facilitated interaction (lund, 1986; rowland & stremel-campbell, 1987). task analysis charts provided the subject with the opportunity to acquire new information slowly and to be provided with small amounts of information at a time required by most pci persons during the introduction of learning tasks (wilson, 1981). the mother's priority skills figure 2 results represent increases between weeks nil to three and weeks nil to six in the priority skills of the mother. when comparing overall percentage increases, the increases in the priority skills of the mother were not as high as those of the subject. however, figure 5 results, which represent high increases in both positive responses (identification, recognition and positive response to communication attempts of the subject) and concrete and verbal positive reinforcement, are significant. increases in positive responses and concrete and verbal positive reinforcement suggest that the training of the mother in the use of positive reinforcement was not only effective, but most likely contributed to increases in positive responses. the use of positive responses were not directly trained and could therefore possibly be viewed as a "spill-over" effect of positive reinforcement training. the significance of these results is underlined by the view that for the pci person, partner responsiveness is known to encourage and facilitate interest in and willingness to participate in communication interaction (girolametto et al., 1986). % c h a n g e 2 0 0 -10717 w e e k 0 w e e k 2 w e e k 3 w e e k β —— s e t t i n g t a b l e ~ ^ m a k i n g b e d * — f e e d i n g cat figure 4: subject: communication skills trends die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwy kings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 ilze pansegrouw and erna alant comparison in priority skills : the subject and the mother significantly fewer overall increases in the priority skills of the mother, when compared to those of the subject, are seen in figure 2. when adding the overall increases of the mother as presented in figure 2 (47% in weeks 0-3 and 53% in weeks 0-6 for communication skills and 105% in weeks 0-3 and 99% in weeks 0-6 for skills at training the subject), only a slight change (50%) in mother's communication behaviour over the six week intervention period is seen. the slower tempo of change of the mother could be viewed against the backdrop of the "set" of table management strategies that have developed over years in order to meet and cope with the daily demands of the pci person. due to limitation in formal and informal societal assistance, families develop rigid/stable strategies which are instrumental in maintaining environmental predictability, security and familiarity (see discussion of homeostasis under proposed intervention model). during the study a few of the family's rules and strategies were observed and mentioned by the mother. it follows that despite mother's willingness to participate in the study and to implement intervention strategies, demands to change management and coping strategies that have provided security and predictability over years, would require a much longer period of professional and social support. however, when looking at figure 2, it is seen that the communication skill of the mother was the only priority skill that showed an increase in week nil to six (after intervention withdrawal). a possible explanation for this could be found when comparing figure 4, 5 and 6. in figure 4 the highest scores during weeks nil to three for the three selected action routines are seen for "set the table" (in which the subject had most prior intervention experience), second highest for "make the bed" (less prior experience than setting table) and third highest for "feed the cat" (least prior experience). after withdrawal of in% c h a n g e — 86 — 6 0 47 47 sstl ν • h i -4 c . p / r v . p / r p / r l / 8 " w i 9 _ 8 0 r p / c t / u η w e e k 3 s i ! w e e k 6 key: c.p/r = concrete positive reinforcement; v.p/r = verbal positive reinforcement; p/r positive responses to communication of subject; l/s = looks at subject; u/s = uses symbols; i/r = ignore responses to communication of subject; p/c = physical contact; t/u = total utterances tervention (weeks nil to six), except for "feed the cat", results show a decline. figure 6 results indicate that, except for "set the table", mother's communication skills increased from weeks nil to six. sub-skills of mother's communication skills that increased at weeks nil to six (figure 5) were "looks at subject", "physical contact" and "total utterances". ignore responses were significantly less in weeks nil to six. these results indicate that for the subject and his mother a relationship existed between the subject's extent of experience and independence at action routines and the communication skills of the mother. it seemed that, as could be expected, increased independence of the subject at performing action routines, lessened the need for "supportive" communication from the mother and vice versa. conclusions the pci population is distinguished by it's heterogenicity and differences are the rule rather than the exception (yoder & villarruel, 1988). a single case design was selected and no generalisations on the basis of these results, can be made. however, it appeared that overall increases in the results of the priority skills of both the mother and the subject could be ascribed to the implementation of communication intervention. a significant decrease in the results of three of the four priority skills after the withdrawal of intervention, strengthens the aforementioned presumption. when taking into account that the results of the pci subject showed the highest increases overall, the most important conclusion points to the dynamic (vs static) nature of the subject's clinical features. when comparing results at the different measurements, motivation seemed to be a key determinant of the features of the priority skills of the subject as well as the communication skills of the mother. results indicated the necessity to target and provide positive reinforcement as well as consequent opportunities to perform skills together with a familiar person in the environment, in order to maintain the pci subject's motivation and willingness regarding intervention aims. the use of domestic action routines and the introduction and representation thereof by means of picture symbol task analysis charts, seem to have succeeded in the facilitation of interaction between the mother and the subject. results which indicated that increased subject independence (at action routines) reduced the need for "supportive" communication from the mother, point to the need for flexibility regarding the selection and period that the % change w e e k 6 figure 5: mother: selected sub-skills of communication skills and skills at training the subject (all action routines) • s e t t i n g table * m a k i n g bed " feeding c a t figure 6: mother: communication skills trends the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication intervention in an adolescent with profound cognitive impairment and autistic features subject is expected to perform each action routine. although in this study the action routine choice of the mother was used, mother's choices should be alternated with those of the subject. additionally the possibilities of increasing the number o f routines (alternating familiar and less familiar routines) or shortening the period of performance of each to less than a week per action routine, could be considered. due to the widely accepted acknowledgement of responsiveness as the facilitator of motivation and willingness to interact, increases in the mother's use of positive responses to the communication o f the subject (possibly indicating a side-effect of positive reinforcement training), are viewed as significant. however, the slower tempo of change in the communication skills of the mother highlighted the need for a realistic and sensitive attitude towards social and historical aspects, daily stressors and general needs of the mother. demands to change coping strategies and communication behaviours that were developed over years, necessitates a prolonged period of professional and social support. additional sources of support could include the active involvement of other family members as well as societal support. results of communication intervention with the pci subject indicated that these persons cannot be denied communication intervention on grounds of clinical features , or psychometric classification alone. poor progress during intervention does not indicate that the person is unable to benefit from intervention and therefore requires only physical care. to provide for the right of the pci person to develop and maintain a quality o f life equal to that of mainstream society, communication intervention must be aimed at the development o f social integration through the facilitation of functional communication and independence skills. the pci person forms one component of the environment in which he spends most of his time. for this reason primary caregiver skill development and sensitivity towards the environment cannot be overemphasised. references alant, e. & emmet, t. (1995). breaking the silence: communication and education for children with severe handicaps. human sciences research council, pretoria. brown, l., nisbet, j., ford, α., sweet, m., shiraga, b, york, j. & loomis, r. (1983) the critical need for non-school instruction in educational programs for severely handicapped students. journal of the association for the severely handicapped, 8 (3), 71-77. ! calculator, s.n. (1988a). teaching functional skills in nonspeaking adults with mental retardation. in s.n. calculator & j.l. bedrosian (eds). communication assessment and intervention for adults with mental retardation. boston, mass : college-hill. calculator, s.n. (1988b). promoting the acquisition and generalisation of conversational skills by individuals with severe disabilities. augmentative and alternative communication. 2(4), 94-103. carlson, f. (1985). picsyms categorical dictionary. baggeboda press, rhode island. st., lawrence, k.s. conti-ramsden, g. (1985). mothers in dialogue with languageimpaired children. topics in language disorders, 58-67. u u n n , l.m. (1965). peabody picture vocabulary test. circle pines, minn.: ags. durarit, m. (1990). severe behaviour problems. new york, the uuilford press. khren, b.j. & lenz, b.k. (1989). adolescents with language disorders: special considerations in providing academically relevant language intervention. seminars in speech and language, 3 (10), 192-203. 1 eshilian, l., haney, m. & falvey, m.a..(1989). domestic skills in m.a. falvey (ed.). community-based curriculum. baltimore, maryland: brookes. girolametto, l.e., greenberg, j. & manolson, h.a. (1986). developing dialogue skills: the hanen early language parent program. seminars in speech and language 4(7), 367-379. gottlieb, m.l. (1989). the response of families to language disorders in the young child. seminars in speech and language 9(1), 47-53. griffiths, r. (1984). griffiths mental developmental scales. bucks: association for research in infant and child development. grover, v. (1990). psycho-educational aspects of mental handicap. in s. lea & d. foster (eds). perspectives on mental handicap in south africa. durban: butterworth. haney, m. & falvey, m.a. (1989). instructional strategies. in m.a. falvey (ed). community-based curriculum. baltimore, maryland: brookes. haring, t.g. (1991). social relationships. in l.h. meyer, c.a. peck & l. brown (eds). critical issues in the lives of people with severe disabilities. baltimore, maryland: brookes. helm, d.t. & kozloff, m.a. (1986). research on parent training: shortcomings and remedies. journal of autism and developmental disorders, 16(1), 1-16. hollander, m. & wolfe, d.a. (1973). nonparametric statistical methods. new york: john wiley & sons. johnson, j. & koegel, r.l. (1982). behavioural assessment and curriculum development. in r.l. koegel, a. rincover & a.l. egel (eds). educating and understanding autistic children. calif.: college-hill. kirchener, d.m. (1991). using verbal scaffolding to facilitate conversational participation and language acquisition in children with pervasive developmental disorders. journal of childhood communication disorders, 14(1), 81-98. koordineringskomitee: jaar van gestremde persone. (1987). gestremdheid in die republiek van suid-afrika: behandeling. vol. 3. pretoria: departement van nasionale gesondheid en bevolkingsontwikkeling. lea, s. (1990). psycho-social aspects of mental handicap. in s. lea & d. foster (eds). perspectives on mental handicap in south africa. durban: butterworth. lund, n.j. (1986). family events and relationships: implications for language assessment and intervention. seminars in speech and language 7(4), 415-429. owens, r.e. & rogerson, b.s. (1988). adults at the presymbolic level. in s.n. calculator & j.l. bedrosian (eds). communication assessment and intervention for adults with mental retardation. boston, mass.: college-hill. pansegrouw, i. (1994). kommunikasie-intervensie by 'n kognitief erg gestremde adolessent met outistiese trekke. ongepubliseerde m.log verhandeling,.universiteit van pretoria. rowland, c. & stremel-campbell, k. (1987). share and share alike; conventional gestures to emergent language for learners with sensory impairments. in l. goetz, d. guess & k. stremelcampbell (eds). innovative program design for individuals with dual sensory impairments. baltimore, maryland: brookes. rustin, l. & ruhr, a. (1989). social skills and the speech impaired. london: taylor & francis. sailor, w. & guess, d. (1983). severely handicapped students: an instructional design. boston, mass.: houghton mifflin. siegel-causey, e. & downing, j.e. (1987). nonsymbolic communication development: theoretical concepts and educational strategies. in l. goetz, d. guess & k. stremel-campbell (eds). innovative program design for individuals with dual sensory impairments. baltimore, maryland: brookes. singer, g.h.s. & irvin, l.k. (1991). supporting families of persons with severe disabilities: emerging findings, practices and questions. in l.h. meyer, c.a. peck, & l. brown (eds). critical issues in the lives of people with severe disabilities. baltimore, maryland: brookes. subcommittee on mental retardation. (1987a). dsm-iii-r classification: axes 1 and 2 categories and codes. in the american psychiatric association. diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (3r d ed., rev.). cambridge: the press syndicate, university of cambridge. walker, m. (1972). signs for makaton. earo. walker, m. (1985). symbols for makaton. earo. wilson, a. (1981). curriculum selection for the deaf-blind; severely/profoundly handicapped. in s.r. walsh & r. holzberg (eds). understanding and educating the deaf-blind i severely d l e suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 ilze pansegrouw and erna alant and profoundly handicapped: an international perspective. the severely handicapped child. in n.j. lass, l.v. mcreynolds, springfield, illinois: charles c. thomas. j.l. northern & d.e. yoder (eds). handbook of speechyoder, d.e. & villarruel, f. (1988). effective communication for language pathology and audiology. philadelphia: b.c. decker. appendix 1: grouping of sub-skills of priority skills of the subject and his mother 1. mother: 1. total utterances (+) communication skills 2. total commands (+) 3. repetitive commands (+) 4. average repetitions (+) 5. physical contact (+) 6. looks at subject (+) 7. response to communication of subject: 7.1 positive (+) 7.2 negative (-) 7.3 ignore (-) 8. uses symbols (+) 9. positive reinforcement: 9.1 concrete (+) 9.2 physical (+) 9.3 verbal (+) 9.4 smile (+) 2. mother: skills at 1. uses symbols (+) training the subject 2. positive reinforcement: 2.1 concrete (+) 2.2 physical (+) 2.3 verbal (+) 2.4 smile (+) 3. subject: 1. looks at mother (+) communication skills 2. smiles (+) 3. smiles for recognition (+) 4. initiates physical contact (+) 5. intentional communication (+) 6. picture symbol communication (+) 7. requests recognition/confirmation during action routines (+) 8. protests (-) 9. anticipates concrete, positive reinforcement (+) 10. ignores commands (-) 1 11. distractible (-) 4. subject: independence 1. performs commands without assistance from mother (+) | 2. performs sub-components of action routines independent/ without commands from mother (+) ι 3. looks at symbols (+) > 4. communicates through symbols (+) 1 5. requests recognition/confirmation for performance of action routines (+) 6. protests (-) 7. anticipates concrete positive reinforcement (+) 8. ignores commands (-) 9. distractible (-) 10. mother performs own commands (-) χ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) communication intervention in an adolescent with profound cognitive impairment and autistic features appendix 2: grid for recording and analysing data of the mother's skills at training the adolescent task se ; the ta ble make the bed feed the cat average week 0 1 2 3 6 0 2 3 6 0 3 6 3 6 measurement β 1 2 3 β 1 2 3 β 1 2 time (min) 8 9 9 10 2 12 4 2 4 9 5 mother mother mother (*) 1 use symbols 16 11 6 5 12 5 7 2 11 7 3 1.1 count/minute 2 1.222 0.666 0.5 6 0.416 1.75 1 2.75 0.777 0.6 1.2 % change 0 -38.8 -66.6 -93 -75 0 -93.0 -70.8 -.83.3 0 -71.7 -78.1 -78 -79 2. positive reinforcement 2.1 concrete 0 7 5 6 0 5 5 5 0 9 7 revised count 1 8.3 6.13 7.25 1 11 7 6 1 11.25 8.25 count/minute 0.125 0.903 0.681 0.725 0.5 0.916 1.75 3 0.25 1.25 1.65 306 513 % change 0 622.6 444.8 267 480 0 83.33 250 500 0 400 560 2.2 physical 1 2 3 2 1 1 2 0 1 1 0 count/minute 0.125 0.222 0.333 0.2 0.5 0.083 05 0 0.25 0.111 0 % change 0 77.77 166.6 257 60 0 -83.3 0 -100 0 -55.5 -100 67 -47 2.3 verbal 2 8 10 7 1 3 5 3 1 4 3 count/minute 0.25 0.888 1.111 0.7 0.5 0.25 1.25 1.5 0.25 0.444 0.6 % change 0 i 255.5 344.4 433 180 0 -50 150 200 0 77.77 140 220 173 2.4 smile 1 1 1 0 0 1 2 0 1 1 0 revised count 1 • 7 4 1 count/minute 1 0.125 0.111 0.111 0 0.5 0.583 1 0.5 0.25 0.111 0 % change 0 | -11.1 -11.1 -11 -100 0 16.66 100 0 0 -55.5 -100 11 -66 average 0 ' 181' 176 171 109 0 -25 86 103 0 59 84 105 99 d e suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the sklar aphasia scale in a foreign language marion fredman, m a . (rand) haifa, israel testing an adult aphasic patient is a complicated procedure liable to, depress and frustrate the patient if not handled correctly. testing a bilingual or polyglot adult aphasic is fraught with added difficulties. apart from the inconsistent responses which a patient may give due to severity of illness or confusion, the bilingual or polyglot patient may be faced with responding to questions in a language with which he was not completely familiar prior to illness. no test can replace extensive observation by a trained clinician. but a suitably designed test provides a starting point for observation and a guide to planning therapy. in a study carried out recently in haifa, israel (fredman2), the writer was faced with the problem of testing 20 adult aphasic patients in hebrew which was not their primary language. some patients had only had a scant knowledge of hebrew prior to illness. one patient was illiterate and others had only been literate in their home language .(table i). thus an aphasic test had to be selected which would lend itself to translation and adaptation into the hebrew language, and yet would not provide long or difficult passages for reading. the sklar aphasic scale (sas). was chosen because it aims at testing ". . . over learned functional communication skills" (sklar7). this scale was developed by dr. maurice sklar at wadsworth veterans hospital in lbs angeles, california, and studies have been reported establishing the validity of the scale, sklar6 and mccloud3. the sas attempts j o assess overt residual language behaviour and is based on the communication theory which ". . . affirms that a social communication occurs when a sign is (1) decoded (received and comprehended), (2) transcoded (transformed internally), and (3) encoded (transmitted orally or graphically)" (sklar7). thus, the sas material is organized into four subtests: (1) auditory decoding: this includes items measuring auditory verbal comprehension and the patient is only required to respond with a motor response or gesture. (2) visual decoding: this subtest attempts to test the patient's ". . . ability to recognize and analyse different types of graphic signs usually acquired with formal learning" (sklar7). (3) oral encoding: items previously presented for decoding are now presented for naming. the patient is also required to describe a picture and read a short news item. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 8,desember971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) marion fredman δ.a ' 1 1 i f ε ρ >s ^ a ο c >2 s «v, •β ε β a to β ε ο i s'o" ρ 53 3 ό ε ο ε 3 η ε ε ..s3 -22 < < < < < < εο-ί §>< c o < £ < < < < < ο > > > > > > | | > ί > > ί > > > > > ; > ε ϋ ϋ ϋ ϋ ϋ ϋ η μ ϋ η ϋ ϋ ω υ ϋ ύ υ υ υ ω .a § ο s s §·ο _ q β η -1 — ~ c ca .3 .2 fc 3 rt 3 η 3"o ca^ps-s 3 3 d o υ cu 3 cu 3 3 (§ £ 3 £ a (2 a < ο 3 ^ * ο (2 ο λ * a * < (n m (n f̂i <«sj psj ^ tjv. £ λ _ ^ ^ ό 3 <υ <υ β 3 3 h i s 3 3 ra ra ιϊ >5 « ι ϊ ι ϊ pq j ffi 2 j ffi κ o<£ ? eg uj3 (λ ο 3 ι-ο .v u γ/1 μ) · <= e 2 ε s s so ο c ω fg c" 0) υ υ p>t3 o o o o o o o " 0 0 0 0 0 0 0 3 o o o o o o o g cisiei!s.eieiei? · c ο « 5 co co co co co co co « π co co co j2 j2 £ co _g ϊί co co ulhlhlhl-ih^ce ctfctfctfctfctfcqctflh ε ε ε ε ε ε ε a c c ' c h c c c ^ d-ohd-ohohd-o,— i a a a 1 » 1 » s 1 ^ § a a <3s ο th c 3 ο υ c ο t3 c <υ ο. χ ά' w α ζ <3 ε k. •s. -ο 5 journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. , december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sklar aphasia scale in foreign language (4) graphic encoding: this tests the patient's ability to write items presented in previous subtests, to write about the picture presented in subtests 2 and 3, and to write progressively longer sentences dictated by the examiner. scoring is as follows: when a patient is able to respond without difficulty he is scored "o" indicating no impairment; if he requires some assistance he is scored "1" and no response or an incorrect response is scored as "2". sklar considers a score of "1" as a difficulty in transcoding ability. each subtest has a possible impairment score of 100 and a prognosis of retraining potential is made on the basis of the total impairment score obtained by dividing the sum of all the scores by 4. case 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 sas* lest 372 326 360 304 294 318 316 242 158 174 208 192 124 224 44 44 58 116 retest 374 204 256 296 88 178 172 148 90 142 146 172 26 42 36 24 16 119 sas test 372 326 360 304 294 318 316 242 158 174 208 192 124 224 44 44 58 116 362 364 retest 374 204 256 296 88 178 172 148 90 142 146 172 26 42 36 24 16 119 318 376 l.m.t.a. test retest 412 341 420 289 305 420 298 256 173 195 365 200 147 221 78 125 88 145 405 242 263 261 120 229 177 160 96 155 239 158 70 79 68 67 76 86 h.l.q. test 84 53 64 68 69 79 63 33 55 55 44 48 46 31 24 21 22 79 80 8 retest 85 54 27 59 24 41 44 39 21 29 42 34 21 22 26 20 20 21 74 78 * note· cases no. 1 and 2 as shown in table i are presented as no. 19 and 20 as they have no l.m.t.a. results. in calculating the r„ between the sas and h.l.q. they again appear as no. 1 and 2. table ii: raw scores because of the choice of test items in the sas, and the fact that it tests functional language ability it was found to be an easy test both to translate and to administer. the sas does not probe as deeply into the patient's linguistic problems as do tests such as the l.m.t.a. (wepman-jones8) or m.t.d.d.a. (schuell4), but it is for this very reason that the writer found it suitable for patients whose knowledge of the test language had not been extensive prior to illness. the passages provided to test reading comprehension are simple newspaper articles which the average israeli patient is used to reading. the objects chosen for naming are known ones in israel and were not tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 18,desember971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) m a r i o n fredman 6 sas sas l.m.t.a. h.l.q. case 1 2 3 4 5 6 1 8 test retest test retest test • retest test retest 1 ] 1 3 3 3 1 4.5 3 2 3 4 2 1 5 4 2.5 2 3 2 3 1 2 1.5 2 2.5 1 4 6 2 5 6 8 3 1 5 5 7 13 .4 . 5 6 11 12 12 6 4 5 8 4 1.5 6 8 4 7 5 • 6.5 9 1.5 7 7 7 14 8 8 8 6 7 9 8 6 8' ' 9 13 12 · 7 8.5 13 12 4.5 6 10 12 10 10 10 12 10 9 9 11 10 9 15. 14 4 5 16 17 12 11 6.5 .14 12 11 9 10.5 11 13 14 16 12 11 14 16 10.5 7 14 9 14 13 8.5 10 14 15 10 15 17.5 15 16 18 18 17 13 17 16 17.5 17 11 16 16 18 14 15 17 16 18 19.5 17 17 15 17 13 ' 18 ' 15 11 19.5 19 15 13 18 19.5 19 — — 18 20 — — 20 19.5 20 — — 17 13 — 19 17 table iii: rank scores test retest (1) anrf (5) ο/ λαιν scores (2) and (6) of raw scores case case di d? 1 di d 2 1 2 4 0 0 2 2 4 0 0 3 0.5 0.25 1 1 4 2 4 1 1 5 1 1 2 4 6 2.5 6.25 1 1 7 2 4 0 . 5 0.25 8 1 1 0 0 9 0 0 0 0 10 0 0 0 0 11 6 36 4 16 12 0 0 2 . 5 / 6.25 ,' o 13 0 0 0 / 6.25 ,' o 14 1 1 0 0 15 0 . 5 0.25 2 4 16 1.5 2.25 1 1 17 1 1 3' 9 18 0 0 2 4 test: r 8 = 0.93 ρ > 0.01 retest: r 3 = 0.95 ρ > 0.01 table iv: r„ between sas and l.m.t.a. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 18, december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) -sklar aphasia scale in foreign language ' changed for the purpose of translation. the divisions into subtests made it easy to omit items when the patients had been illiterate in hebrew. nevertheless, it was considered desirable to use scores obtained on the l.m.t.a. by 18 out of the 20 patients in order to provide a control, and indicate validity of the sas scores in hebrew. the results on the two tests and retests (table ii and iii) were ranked and the spearman rank correlation coefficient r8 was used to establish the correlation between the results on the two tests. a formula taking into account ties in ranking was used as described by siegel5. the r on the sas and l.m.t.a. test results was 0.93 with a level of significance of ρ = 0 . 0 1 (table iv). on the retests the , obtained was 0.95 at a level of significance of p = 0.01 (table iv). as the level of significance between the sas and l.m.t.a. is high, it may be concluded that the two tests measure the same disorder. the, l.m.t.a. was proved valid as a test of aphasia in hebrew, by bar-david1, therefore it was assumed by case. di d? 1 1 1 . 5 2.25 2 0 . 5 0.25 3 1 . 5 2,25 4 4 16 5 8 64 6 0 0 7 2 4 8 0 0 9 2.5 •6.25 10 1 1 11 1 1 12 3.5 12.25 13 1.5 2.25 14 2 . 4 15 3 9 16· 3 9 17 2.5 6.25 18 1.5 2.25 19 2 4 20 2 4 r s = 0.88 ρ > 0.01 table v: r s between sas test and h.l.q. test ((3) and (7) of raw scores) the writer that the sas was also valid. it should be noted that the "tell a story" item of the l.m.t.a. was omitted in the comparison of the two tests on the advice of wepman9 who maintained that there was no comparable item in the sas. the patients used in this study came from ten different countries and spoke seven languages. their ages ranged from 34 to 76 and their educational status from illiterate to university degree (table i). tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 18, desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) m a r i o n f r e d m a n 9* —> ό a s s s a a a a a a a ^ivo^oomomovoojq^moovo^oov ι—ι γπ ιη «λ ιλ"λ «λ β s •ο ηνονοο^ο^ονο'λ^-^'λμομ'λμ • oh 1 oh 1 ootto^oo^r^^rnooor^fnr-)'—ι^^ο^^ο^ •λ ιλ ιλ «λ l/"> l/">l/"> ·/">·/"> ·/"> •o' o) ε !/-> «λ «λ !/-> ^ifsj^ m ixrnrnrqt^^ttov^r^vottttoooovt '̂-jvopn • τ in in i/-> m ι/-> «λ -ν § << l/-> !/•>«/"> «λ «λ «λ •ο j 1—1 1—1 1—1 i—l 1—1 1—1 (n | ι/-> «λ «λ sa s -ν ε artnoo -̂ncsomvî f-vomoovot̂ ono^ α << ^ ^ cncn ^^(s^fommm i 1—(1—(1—(1—(1— <3 1—(1—(1—(1—(1— ο ο vi & (ν " β u κ •ο 3 α ^ * 3 co β σ s ο 8 8 5 = 2$ 8 8 η ο § 2 /isfg ' τ3 ·ο η ω ο ο ω ω .3 ο -μ s ο α λ 3 ϊτ -cm journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. is, december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sklar aphasia scale in foreign language as the purpose of the study carried out in israel was to establish the influence of therapy in hebrew on the home language of the bilingual or polyglot adult aphasic, a test in the patient's home language was also required. a home language questionnaire was devised which consisted of 20 questions to test activities which the patient's family could observe. the patient was tested in his home language and the sas before and after three months' treatment in hebrew. the correlation between the" home language questionnaire and the-sas is shown in table v. where improvement was made on the sas a similar improvement was indicated in the patient's home language (table vi). thus it may be seen that the sas proved to be a useful evaluating instrument in patients who had spoken a variety of languages prior to illness. this writer is of the opinion that the sas would be suitable in other languages in many parts of the world. summary the sklar aphasia scale was used to test 20 bilingual and polyglot aphasic patients in a project carried out in haifa, israel. the test was compared to the l.m.t.a. on 18 of the patients and proved to be valid. this writer believes that it is an easily translatable, adaptable scale which could be used for aphasics in other parts of the world where it is impractical to test the patient in his home language. opsomming die sklar afasieskaal is gebruik om 20 tweeen veeltalige afasiese gevalle te toets in haifa, israel. die toets is vergelyk teen die ..language modality test of aphasia" wat op 18 van die proefpersone uitgevoer is en dit het geblyk geldig te wees. die skrywer glo dat hierdie 'n maklik vertaalbare en aanpasbare skaal is wat gebruik kan word vir afasiegevalle in ander dele van die wereld waar dit onprakties of onmoontlik is om die geval in sy huistaal te toets. acknowledgement this study was supported in part by public health service research grant no. 06-12 1134-108-13 from the bureau of disease prevention and environment control of the united states government. the writer acknowledges with thanks the assistance given by dahlia bar-david in providing the l.m.t.a. scores for this study. references: 1 bar-david, d. (1971): adaptation of the wepman-jones language modalities test for aphasia into hebrew. j. communication disorders, vol. 4, 2 fredman, m. (1970): the effect of therapy given in another language, on the home language of the bilingual or polyglot adult aphasic. m.a. dissertation, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse verenigingvir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 18, desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ^ m a r i o n fredman 3 mccloud e. (1962): comparison of four aphasia tests administered to ?2 penons chapter 4 of m.a. thesis, umversity of california 4 schuell η (1965): administrative manual for the minnesota test for diff^ntial diagnosis of aphasia. university of minnesota press, minneapolis. 5 siegel, s. (1956): non-parametric statistics for the behavioural sciences. mcgraw-hill book co., inc. 6 sklar μ (1963): relations of psychological and language test scores and autopsy findings in aphasia. j. speech hearing research, vol. 6, no. 1. 7. sklar, m. (1966): sklar aphasia manual. western psychological services, california. 8 wepman, j. m., jones, l. v., bock, r. d. and van pelt, d. (1961): manual for language modality test of aphasia. university of chicago, education industry service. 9. wepman, j. m. (1969): personal correspondence. journal of the south african speech and hearing association,· vol. is, december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 43 ouerleiding by ouers wat swart babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte corrieta campbell & brenda louw departement kommunikasiqjatologie universiteit van pretoria opsomming daar is aanduidings dat die oners vangesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas dikwels behoφe toon aan meer inligting omtrent hul baba segeboortedrfek as wat aan hulle verskafword. in suid-afiika bestaan daar egter nog 'n leemte in die literatuur omtrent die spesifieke behoeftes van die ouers van 'n swart baba met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte. in hierdie studie is daar eerstens 'n bepaling gedoen van die ouers van swart gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas se unieke behoφes en tweedens is daar, op grond van die verkree resultate, 'n inligtingstuk vir hierdie teikengroep saamgestel. op grond van die resultate verkry met behulp van die vyftien proefpersone wat aan hierdie studie deelgeneem het, is daar eerstens bevind dat die ouers van swartgesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas in suid-afrika eenderssoortige behoeftes as ouers van babas met dieselfde aangeboregebrek wereldwyd vertoon. tweedens is daar bevind dat die inligtingstuk wat as deel van die studie saamgestel is, 'n positiewe bydrae gelewer het tot die ouerleiding wat met die proefpersone in die studie uitgevoer is. die resultate hou dus ook belangrike terapeutiese implikasies in. abstract there are indications that the parents of babies with a cleft up and palate often require more information regarding their baby's birth defect than what is given to them. in south africa shortcomings exist in the literature covering the specific requirements of parents of black cleft lip and palate babies. in this study the unique requirements of parents of black cleft lip and palate babies was determined and, based on these results, an information pamphlet was prepared for this population group. based on the results obtained with the help of fifteen participants it was established, firstly, that the needs of black south-arfican parents of children with a cleft lip and palate are similar to the needs of parents of babies with the same birth defect world-wide. secondly, it was established that the information pamphlet which was compiled as part of the study made a positive contribution to parent guidance conducted with the participants. important therapeutic implications were also reflected by the results. < tot op hede het die.swart"gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltepopulasie in suid-afrika weinig belangstelling uitgelok (louw, 1986). bestaande navorsing oor die problematiek van hierdie aangebore gebrek is slegs op geselekteerde populasies uitgevoer. daar is op internasionale vlak 'n behoefte aan navorsing en kultureel aangepaste intervensieprogramme oor die problematiek van die gesplete lip en verhemelte binne anderskleurige populasies en dit neem voortdurend toe (toliverweddington, 1990). vroee intervensie, soos wat tans suksesvol in die blanke populasie van suid-afrika deur middel van ouerleiding toegepas word, kan moontlik die oplossing wees vir die uitgesproke behoefte aan hulp en leiding onder die swart bevolking (bzoch, 1989; brookshire, lynch & fox, 1980; louw, 1988). die implementering van vroee intervensie word voorgestaan omdat swart gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltepasiente soms glad nie by 'n hospitaal uitkom voordat chirurgiese sluiting van die baba se lip en verhemelte plaasvind nie. hierdie pasiente is ook dikwels buite bereik van 'n spraakterapeut nadat hulle die hospitaal verlaat het. swart ouers van gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas in die rsa vertoon 'n unieke kultuur, taal en sosio-emosionele omstandighede. die vraag ontstaan dus of dit nie ook 'n unieke wyse van ouerleiding impliseer nie. kan die bestaande inligting oor ouerleiding van blanke gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas sonder meer op hul swart ewekniee toegepas word? die belang van interkulturele veranderlikes in hulpverlening word tans sterk in die spraakheelkunde beklemtoon. wereldwyd begin spraakheelkundige organisasies hulself ten doel stel om kultureel diverse populasies ten voile te akkommodeer. daar is reeds in die vsa wetgewing in di£ verband ingestel (taylor, 1986). watter unieke kulturele kenmerke en behoeftes kom by die swart suid-afrikaanse bevolking voor? eerstens is kommunikasieprobleme in kruis-kulturele gesondheidsdienste 'n probleem. daar word tien verskillende swart tale in suidafrika gepraat. engels is dikwels 'n tweede of derde taal vir die swart suid-afrikaner. in 'n studie wat deur louw (1986) geloods is oor swart gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas het 76% van die moeders min of geen engels verstaan nie en moes daar van 'n tolk gebruik gemaak word om effektiewe interaksie en oordrag te bewerkstellig. tweedens, ouers wat afkomstig is uit meer tradisionele en ongeletterde agtergronde, neig om kulturele gebruike en bygelowe aan te hang (smoot, kucan, cope en aase, 1988). dabuto-brown (1989) doen verslag dat die tipiese afrika-kultuur 'n baba met 'n erge wanformasie van die gesig, as onaanvaarbaar beskou. derdens, 'n aspek wat wetenskaplik moeilik peilbaar is, th south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39 1992 sas 1 2 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 omdat dit in die westerse kultuur as 'n kriminele oortreding beskou word en dus selde aangemeld word, is kindermoord. strauss (1985) bevind dat dit dikwels binne 'n derde wereldsamelewing as 'n ritueel of tradisionele prosedure beskou word om 'n baba wat met 'n kongenitale afwyking gebore is om die lewe te bring. verder noem hy dat kinders wat met 'n kongenitale defek gebore word, dikwels as 'n slegte voorbode deur die familie beskou word en dus weggesteek of verwaarloos word. binne die suid-afrikaanse konteks word die kliniese waarneming gemaak dat 'n groot persentasie van die swart bevolking oningelig is met betrekking tot die beskikbare mediese ondersteuningsdienste vir 'n baba met gesplete lip en verhemelte. dit kan daartoe lei dat die ouers glad nie aanmeld vir mediese en ander beskikbare vorme van gespesialiseerde behandeling nie of, soos dabuto-brown (1989) aandui, dikwels in 'n baie laat stadium aanmeld vir sodanige hulp vir hulle baba. vierdens, word die voeding van swart babas met gesplete lip en verhemelte ook deur kulturele gebruike geraak. dit is baie belangrik vir 'n swart moeder om haar baba te borsvoed (louw, 1986). die babas word dikwels suksesvol geborsvoed sonder 'n voedingsplaatjie of enige gespesialiseerde opleiding. hierdie verskynsel kan toegeskryf word aan 'n drang om te oorleef en die beskouing dat die aankoop van 'n plaasvervanger vir moedersmelk, 'n onnodige uitgawe is (louw, 1987). vyfdens en laastens vertoon swart babas met hierdie gebrek ook eiesoortige ontwikkelingskenmerke (louw, 1986). hierdie verskynsel kan moontlik toegeskiyf word aan die feit dat die gemiddelde swart moeder in suid-afrika dalknie die voile implikasies van die afwyking besef nie en eerder met die onmiddellike situasie en behoeftes gemoeid is as om toekomsgerig te wees. die behoeftes van swart babas in suid-afrika wat met 'n gesplete verhemelte gebore word, is dus uniek hierdie waargenome verskille tussen die blanke en swart populasies noodsaak gevolglik aanpassings aan bestaande dienslewering en vroee intervensieprogramme (mcwilliams, morris & shelton, 1984), sodat dit toepaslik sal wees vir swart gesplete-lip-enverhemeltebabas binne die multikulturele opset in suidafrika. vanwee die kompleksiteit van die probleem en die nadelige invloed wat 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte op 'n kind se ontwikkeling kan uitoefen (fox, lynch & brookshire, 1978), beveel outoriteite aan dat vroee intervensie vir 'n gesplete-lip-enverhemeltebaba binne spanverband geskied (bzoch, 1989; krogman, 1979). macdonald (1978) meen dat die ouers beskou moet word as baie belangrike lede van die gesplete-lipen-verhemeltespan. die ouers speel ook by vroee ingryping 'n kardinale rol. die doel van vroee ingryping is eerstens voorkomend en tweedens fassiliterend. die spraakterapeut bereik die kind indirek deur sy ouers te lei. suksesvolle ouergesentreerde terapie bestaan uit drie komponente, naamlik inligtingverskaffing (wat weet die ouers?), beraad (hoe voel die ouers?) en opleiding (wat moet die ouers doen?). die wyse waarop vroee intervensie en meer spesifiek ouerleiding plaasvind, wissel van mekaar. geskrewe materiaal (inligting), byvoorbeeld pamflette (olmstead, hardwick & fortier, 1980), kan sinvol aangewend word aangesien dit die ouers tyd bied om na te dink oor hulle vrae en die ouers wat buite bereik van 'n spraakterapeut is, van inligting voorsien. aanpassings in die gebruik van geskrewe materiaal moet egter gemaak word wanneer daar met ongeletterde ouers gewerk word. pamflette kan as riglyn dien vir die spraakterapeut om inligting oor te dra. dit word egter in die literatuur beklemtoon dat inligtingverskaffing net 'n onderdeel van 'n ouerleicorrieta campbell & brenda louw dingsprogram uitmaak. die ander twee genoemde aspekte, naamlik beraad en opleiding, moet ook aandag geniet as die span die ouers en baba ten voile wil ondersteun (louw 1988). die bepaling van die spesifieke behoeftes van moeders van swart babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte, wat as basis kan dien vir 'n ouerleidinginligtingstuk, kan dus as vertrekpunt dien vir ouerleiding tydens vroee intervensie met die spesifieke populasie. metode doel die doel van die studie is om 'n effektiewe inligtingstuk oor die gesplete-lip-en-verhemelteafwyking saam te stel wat sosiokultureel toepaslik is vir gebruik deur die plaaslike swart bevolking in suid-afrika. navorsingsontwerp hierdie studie is in die vorm van 'n tweegroepontwerp, wat 'n tussengroepontwerpmetode is, uitgevoer (smit, 1983). die toetspopulasie bestaan uit twee groepe proefpersone, wat op toevallige wyse geselekteer word. die variansie tussen die twee groepe word ook waargeneem. 'n onafhanklike veranderlike, naamlik 'n inligtingstuk, word by die een groep ingelyf, terwyl dit nie by die ander groep gedoen word nie. tussengroepsvergelykings word getref ten opsigte van die veranderlike, naamlik 'n vraelys, wat deur albei groepe beantwoord word. proefpersoonseleksie die proefpersone was vyftien moeders van swart babas met 'n onherstelde gesplete lip en verhemelte. hulle is op toevallige wyse geselekteer in hospitaalklinieke en in twee groepe verdeel. van groep 1 word inligting in verband met die moeders se kennis omtrent die gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltegebrek, die behoeftes en probleme wat hulle ondervind, asook die moeder se emosionele belewenis van haar baba verkry. op grond van die inligting soos verkiy van groep 1 is daar 'n inligtingstuk saamgestel. groep 2 word benut om die effektiwiteit van hierdie inligtingstuk te evalueer. proefpersone moes aan 'n suidafrikaanse swart groep behoort aangesien die studie gemik is op die saamstel van 'n inligtingstuk vir die moeders van swart suid-afrikaanse babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte. die babas moes ten tye van die uitvoering van die studie nog geen chirurgiese behandeling vir die gesplete lip en verhemelte ontvang het nie. die teikenpopulasie van die studie is ouers van jong babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte wat behoefte het aan inligting, beraad en opleiding met betrekking tot die hantering van hulle babas. die babas moes binne die ouderdomsgroep 0 tot 24 maande val. sodoende het 'n beperkte variasie in terme van behoeftes en ontwikkelingmylpale tussen die babas voorgekom (anderson, nelson & fowler, 1978). die babas moes geen ander kongenitale afwyking buiten die gesplete lip en/of verhemelte vertoon nie: sodoende verkiy die steekproef 'n meer homogene aard wat die kenmerke van die babas en die ouers se behoeftes betre'f (mcwilliams et al. 1984). tabel 1 verskaf'n opsommende weergawe van die kenmerke van die proefpersone. / materiaal en apparaat vraelys op grond van reeds gei'dentifiseerde vraagtukke en probleme wat algemeen by ouers van babas met gesplete lip en verhemelte presenteer (massengill & phillips, 1975; macthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouerleiding by ouers van swart babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte 45 tabel 1: algemene becld van die proefpersone wat by hierdie ondersoek be trek is. pp. no. moeder skolasties kind geslag kind ouderdom huistaal tipe spleet* 1 st. 10 v 6 mde n. sotho 1 2 st. 2 v 8 mde shangaan 5 3 st. 1 v 1,5 mde zoeloe 3 4 geen v 8 mde zoeloe 1 5 st. 7 v 7 mde tsonga 2 6 st. 5 v 6 mde swazi 2 7 st. 2 v 5 mde zoeloe 1 8 st. 5 v 8 mde n.sotho 2 9 st. 2 μ 1 md s.sotho 4 10 st. 8 μ 2 mde s.sotho 5 11 st. 3 μ 5 mde zoeloe 4 12 st. 2 v 5 mde s.sotho 2 13 st. 5 v 11 mde tsonga 3 14 st. 2 v 6 mde shangaan 4 15 st. 7 μ 3 mde zoeloe 1 * sleutel tot die klassifisering van tipes splete: (acpa-klassifikasie, bzoch,1989). 1 spleet van die lip en alveolus. 2 spleet van die velum. 3 spleet van die palatum en velum. 4 unilaterale splete van die lip, alveolus, palatum en velum. 5 bilaterale spleet van die lip, alveolus, palatum en velum. donald, 1978; starr, 1983), is daar 'n vraelys ontwerp vir die peiling van proefpersone se emosionele belewenisse en behoeftes en om hul kennis ten opsigte van hul gesplete-lip-enverhemeltebabas te bepaal. om die analise van die navorsingsdata te vergemaklik, is die vrae verdeel in vier kategoriee. op hierdie wyse word daar ook meer struktuur verleen aan die evaluasie-proses (smit, 1983). die kategoriee sluit die volgende in: die moeder se kennis aangaande die kind se afwyking en die moontlike oorsake van die gesplete lip en verhemelte by haar kind word geevalueer. moontlike probleme wat die baba en sy familie kan ondervind as direkte gevolg van sy gesplete lip en verhemelte word ondersoek. aspekte wat gedek word, is: gehoor, voeding, kommunikasiegedrag, finansies en kulturele aanvaarding van die baba. | daar word bepaal van watter behandelings en ondersteuningsdienste die moeder kennis dra. die familie se gevoelens jeens die mediese behandeling word ook ondersoek. laastens word die ouers eri direkte familie van die gespeltelip-en-verhemeltebaba se i emosionele belewenis van die baba, asook die vooruitsigte met betrekking tot opvolgbehandeling nadat die inisiele chirurgie voltooi is, met behulp van die vraelys ondersoek. die navorser voltooi self deur 'n verbale onderhoud, met behulp van 'n tolk indien nodig, die vraelys saam met die moeder van die gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebaba. sodoende word 'n groter mate van eenvormigheid verkry in die wyse waarop die vraelys aangebied word (campbell, 1990). inligtingstuk. 'n inligtingstuk is saamgestel met die doel om te beantwoord aan die moeders van swart babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte se behoefte aan inligting en emosionele ondersteuning, soos reeds gei'dentifiseer tydens die toepassing van die vraelys op die proefpersone in groep 1 (sien proefpersqonseleksie). die inligtingstuk is saamgestel uit reeds bestaande ouerleidingsprogramme, wat spesifiek ontwerp is vir gebruik tydens ouergesentreerde intervensie binne die gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltepopulasie (olmstead, hardwick & fortier, 1980; brookshire, lynch & fox, 1980; macdonald, 1978). die bestaande materiaal is aangepas by die swart bevolking in suidafrika se spesifieke kultuur, kenmerke en behoeftes, soos bepaal deur 'n literatuurstudie (louw, 1986; cole, 1981; cervenka, 1984; dabuto-brown, 1989; rampp, 1984) en deur die resultate van die bogenoemde vraelys. aangesien daar onder die gemiddelde swart bevolking in suid-afrika 'n hoer graad van geletterdheid in engels as in afrikaans blyk te wees, is die inligtingstuk in engels geskryf. daar is gebruik gemaak van prente van swart babas en die inligting is baie konkreet en visueel voorgestel om dit meer toeganklik te maak vir moeders wat nie kan lees nie of nie engels begryp nie. die inligtingstuk kan ook met ongeletterde ouers gebruik word, deurdat die spraakterapeut, met behulp van 'n tolk, die inligting verbaal oordra en toelig met die illustrasies wat in die inligtingstuk verskaf word. die inligtingstuk bestaan uit vyf afdelings, naamlik: die anatomie van die orofasiale strukture en die aard en oorsake van 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte. voeding van 'n gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebaba. kommunikasie en taalstimulasie by 'n gesplete lip-enverhemeltebaba. middeloorprobleme by die baba met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte en die hantering daarvan. die belang van chirurgiese en ortodontiese behandeling vir 'n gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebaba en die belang van gereelde opvolgbehandeling nadat die baba uit die hospitaal ontslaan is. prosedure data-insameling hierdie studie berus op data wat van twee groepe proefpersone verkry is, naamlik, groep 1 en groep 2 (sien proefpersoonseleksie). the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 corrieta campbell insameling van data by groep 1: die proefpersone is by verskeie hospitale en klinieke gekontak en met behulp van 'n opgeleide tolk ondervra. die vraelyste is, met behulp van 'n tolk, mondelings met die proefpersone behandel en deur die navorser self ingevul. daar is doelbewus gepoog om geen leidrade of aanduidinge met betrekking tot die beantwoording van die vrae aan die proefpersone te verskaf nie. die antwoorde watgenoteer is, is uitsluitlik die van die proefpersone. insameling van data van groep 2: die proefpersone in hierdie groep is by dieselfde hospitale en klinieke as in die geval van groep 1 gekontak. die proefpersone is met behulp van 'n tolk bekendgestel aan die inligtingstuk wat op grond van die bevindinge uit groep 1 as vraelyste saamgestel is. daar is sorg geneem om te verseker dat die proefpersone die inligting ten voile begryp en alle vrae voortspruitend daaruit is deur die navorser met behulp van die tolk beantwoord. nadat die proefpersone aan die inligtingstuk bekendgestel is, is dieselfde vraelys as wat met groep 1 uitgevoer is, ongeveer 1 maand later met die hulp van 'n tolk mondelings op die proefpersone in groep 2 toegepas. die proefpersone se antwoorde is deur die navorser self aangeteken. data-analiee die proefpersone se response op die vraelyste is volgens die onderskeie kategoriee waaruit die vraelys bestaan daarna statistics geanaliseer en op grafiese wyse voorgestel. die betroubaarheid van die intergroepverskille is op statistiese wyse bereken (fisher se toets: zar 1984). die statistiese berekeninge is ook gedoen om vas te stel of die inligtingstuk wat aan die proefpersone in groep 2 bekendgestel is, voordat hulle die vraelys beantwoord het, wel 'n beduidende verskil in hul kennis omtrent die oorsake, behandeling en die noodsaaklikheid van opvolgondersoeke teweeggebring het. resultate resultate word weergegee in die vorm van 'n vergelyking tussen groep 1, wat nie aan die inligtingstuk blootgestel was nie, en groep 2 wat wel aan die inligtingstuk blootgestel was, se response op dieselfde vraelys. tabel 2: vergelyking van groep 1 en groep 2 se kennis oor moontlike oorsake van gespelete verhemelte. proefpersone wat kennis dra omtrent die moontlike oorsake van gesplete lip en verhemelte: groep 1 (n=8) groep 2 (n=7) p-waarde 0% 80% 0,01 beduidende verskil soos voorgestel in tabel 2, dra die proefpersone in groep 2 beduidend meer kennis van die moontlike oorsake van 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte as die proefpersone in groep 1. alhoewel daar nie 'n beduidende verskil is tussen die mate waarin die twee groepe proefpersone hul babas aanvaar nie, is dit tog opmerklik dat 100% van die proefpersone in groep 2 geantwoord het dat hulle hul baba aanvaar, terwyl slegs 70% van die proefpersone in groep 1 aandui dat hulle hul baba aanvaar (sien tabel 3). die twee groepe se antwoorde betreffende hul familielede se aanvaarding van die babas toon 'n verskil van tabel 3: vergelyking van groep 1 en groep 2 se aanvaarding van hulle gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas. proefpersone wat hul babas aanvaar: groep 1 (n=8) groep 2 (n=7) p-waarde 70% 100% 0,24 nie-beduidende verskil proefpersone wie se families hul babas aanvaar: groep 1 (n=8) groep 2 (n=7) p-waarde 30% 40% 0,53 nie-beduidende verskil tabel 4: proefpersone se bewustheid van hulp en behandeling beskikbaar vir hul babas. groep 1 (n=8) groep 2 (n=7) p-waarde bewus van chirurgiese herstel van die spleet: 40% 100% 0,01 beduidende verskil bewus van ortodontiese behandeling en voedingsplaatjie: 10% 80% 0,002 beduidende verskil bewus van mediese behandeling van middeloorprobleme: 0% 100% 0,003 beduidende verskil bewus van voedingsen spraakterapie: 10% 100% 0|002 beduidende verskil tabel 5: vooruitsigte van opvolgbesoeke | 1 groep 1 (n=8) groep 2 (n=7) p-waarde 40% 100% p.i beduidende verskil slegs 10% wat te wagte kan wees aangesien slegs die moeders in groep 2 en nie hulle familielede nie, aan die inligtingstuk blootgestel was. volgens tabel 4 is daar ook 'n duidelike verskil tussen groep 1 en 2 se bewustheid van die hulp 'en behandeling wat vir hul babas beskikbaar is. daar was 'n statisties beduidende verhoging van die proefpersone in groep 2 (wat aan die inligtingstuk blootgestel was) se bewustheid van die volgende vier behandelingsmoontlikhede, naamlik chirurgiese herstel van die spleet, ortodontiese behandeling en/of'n voedingsplaatjie, mediese behandeling vir middeloorprobleme en voedingsthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouerleiding by ouers van swart babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte 47 en spraakterapie. laastens vertoon die vooruitsig van opvolgbesoek (sien tabel 5) nadat die babas se inisiele behandeling voltooi is, by die proefpersone in groep 2 ook beduidend beter as by groep 1 al groep 2 se proefpersone antwoord dat hulle sal terugkeer vir verdere behandeling en hulp, terwyl slegs 40% van die moeders in groep 1, wat nie aan die inligtingstuk blootgestel was nie, aandui dat hulle sal terugkeer vir opvolgbehandeling. bespreking van resultate die resultate wat van die proefpersone in groep 1 verkry is en die response van groep 2 op dieselfde vraelys word gesamentlik bespreek. dit moet deurentyd in gedagte gehou word dat groep 1 se response 'n weerspieeling is van die ouers wat nog min of geen ouerleiding on tvang het nie. die proefpersone in groep 2 het egter voor die invul van die vraelys ouerleiding ontvang deur middel van die verbale gebruik van die inligtingstuk wat ook saamgestel is as deel van hierdie studie. die inligtingstuk is gebaseer op die behoeftes, soos aangedui deur groep 1, met die beantwoording van die vraelys. ten spyte daarvan dat die diagnose van hulle kind se gebrek deur mediese personeel aan die ouers oorgedra is, blyk dit duidelik uit die resultate dat nie een van die proefpersone in groep 1 kennis gedra het van die moontlike oorsake van 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte nie. hierdie bevinding korreleer met resultate van 'n soortgelyke ondersoek wat deur slutsky (1969) uitgevoer is op moeders van gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas. daar is bevind dat 76% van die moeders wat ondervra is pas na die geboortes van hul babas nog nooit van 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte gehoor het nie (mcwilliams et al. 1984). mcwilliams et al. (1984) maak die stelling dat hoe meer bekend die ouer van 'n gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebaba met die afwyking is, hoe makliker sal dit vir die ouer wees om daarby aan te pas. die feit dat die proefpersone in groep 1 geen kennis gedra het van 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte as 'n kongenitale afwyking nie, dui daarop dat hulle nie genoegsaam bygestaan is ten opsigte van die aanvaarding en begrip van hul babas se geboorteafwykings nie. die proefpersone in groep i2 se basiese kennis aangaande die aard en oorsake van die igesplete-lip-en-verhemelteverskynsel was beduidend beter as die basiese kennis waaroor die proefpersone in groep 1 beskik het. 80% in groep 2, teenoor geeneen van die proefpersone in groep 1 nie, was bewus van sommige etiologiese faktore wat geassosieer word met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte. die inligtingstuk het dus die ouers in groep 2 gehelp om 'n wye siening te ontwikkel van die aard en moontlike oorsaak van hul baba se geboorte-afwyking. ouers behoort die geleentheid te he 'om inligting oor die moontlike oorsake van 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte te ontvang. sommige ouers is tevrede met bloot algemene inligting, maar ander verlang meer inligting, veral as die afwyking familiaal blyk te wees (siegel, 1979). dit is nie die spraakterapeut se rol om genetiese raadgewing te verskaf aan die ouers nie, maar die moontlikheid van die genetiese aard van die spleet en beskikbaarheid van hierdie diens moet aan hulle oorgedra word. η ondersoek na die probleme wat die gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas sowel as hul ouers ondervind het, is met die oog °p die saamstel van die inligtingstuk, op groep 1 se proefpersone geloods. hul response op die vraelys het getoon dat 70% van die proefpersone (groep 1) of hul babas wel direk as gevolg van die gebrek 'n probleem ondervind het. verder het 30% van die proefpersone meer as een probleem gerapporteer. volgens die resultate van die vraelys was die mees pre valente probleem die van voeding. hierdie bevinding sluit aan by rampp (1984) se bevinding dat die voeding van hul gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebaba 'n algemene bekommernis is by die ouers van hierdie kinders. 87% van die babas wat voedingsprobleme ondervind in hierdie studie het dikwels verstik tydens voeding en 62% van die wat dikwels verstik, kry ook voedsel in die nasale ruimte tydens voeding. mc williams et al. (1984) het bevind dat vroee voedings-ervarings wat 'n baba en ouer ondervind hoogs relevant is vir psigososiale sowel as vir spraaken taalontwikkeling. voedingsprobleme kan oorkom word deur die nodige inligting oor voedingsmetodes en -hulpmiddels aan-die ouers te verskaf en hulle te ondersteun in die uitvoervan hierdie moeilike taak(louw, 1987). slegs 10% van die moeders in groep 1 was bewus van voedingsterapie (verskaf deur 'n spraakterapeut), teenoor 100% van die proefpersone in groep 2 wat wel daarvan bewus was. die moontlikheid dat groep 2 se ouers dus sal hulp vra en sal terugkeer vir opvolgsessies word dus ookgroter as in die geval van groep 1, wat nie aan die inligtingstuk blootgestel was nie. die mediese behandeling vir 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte is 'n voortgesette en duur proses. aangesien die populasie waaruit die proefpersone geselekteer is oorwegend onder die laer inkomstegroep in suid-afrika ressorteer en dikwels nie aan mediese fondse behoort nie, word die moontlikheid van finansiele probleme by die behandeling van die proefpersone se babas nie uitgesluit nie. wat interessant is, is dat die proefpersone in groep 1 se response op die vraelys die teendeel bewys het. 90% van die proefpersone antwoord dat daar geen finansiele probleme rakende die behandeling van hul gesplete-lipen-verhemeltebabas is nie. die rede hiervoor blyk te wees dat die provinsiale hospitale voorsiening maak vir pasiente in die laer inkomstegroep. hospitaalonkoste, konsultasiefooie, mediese behandeling en medikasie word feitlik 100% deur die staat gesubsideer. die antwoorde op die vraelys dui verder aan dat die vervoeronkoste van 40% van die proefpersone in groep 1 ook deur die hospitaal gedek word. ambulanse of treinkaartjies word voorsien aan behoeftige pasiente wat ver van die hospitaal woon. hierdie vervoerbystand het weer positiewe implikasies wanneer opvolgbesoeke moet realiseer. ten spyte daarvan dat die baba 'n baie duur behandelingsproses ondergaan, ontvang die ouers van swart gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas bystand om die finansiele las te verlig. kostefaktore hoef dus nie 'n beperkende faktor in die kind se behandelingsproses te wees nie. die moeders en families se emosionele belewing van die baba met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte en van sy deformiteit is ook met behulp van die vraelys ondersoek. dit is 'n groot skok vir 'n ouer/ouers om onverwags 'n baba met hierdie geboortedefek te aanvaar en te versorg (macdonald, 1978). die navorsingresultate van die proefpersone in groep 1 korreleer met hierdie stelling. die effektiwiteit van die behandelingsproses word beslis bevoordeel wanneer die baba se ouers hom of haar ten voile aanvaar (starr, 1983). dit is dus noodsaaklik dat die spraakterapeut ondersoek instel na die ouers van hierdie babas se emosionele reaksie op hul baba se geboortedefeken aandag daaraan skenk tydens die verskaffing van inligting aan die ouers. deur inligting op 'n gestruktureerde en empatiese wyse aan die ouers te verskaf (verkieslik in hulle moedertaal), kry die ouers nie slegs beter insig in hul baba se afwyking nie, maar word hulle ook gelei na begrip en aanvaarding van hulle eie gevoelens jeens hul baba met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte (louw, 1988). volgens krogmann (1979) is die ouers van die baba belangrike lede van die multi-dissiplinere span. die ouers se samewerking sal noodwendig verbeter wanneer hulle positief voel oor die behandeling wat hulle baba ontvang. slegs die helfte van die proefpersone in groep 1 het positief gevoel oor die the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 48 behandeling wat hul babas gaan ontvang. dit was egter opmerklik dat 80% van di£ wat positief gevoel het, buitepasiente was by die gesplete gesigsdeformiteite kliniekvan die departement kaak-, gesigen mondchirurgie, universiteit van pretoria, waar die ouers nou saamwerk met 'n volledige multidissiplindre span. die uitgangspunt waarop hierdie navorsingsprojek gebaseer is, naamlik dat vroee intervensie binne multi-dissiplinere verband die ouers van jong gesplete-lip-enverhemeltebabas tot voordeel strek, word hierdeur ondersteun (bzoch, 1989). 'n verdere aanduiding dat 'n verandering van gesindheid deur effektiewe ouerleiding teweeggebring kan word, is die verskynsel dat 100% van die proefpersone in groep 2 (wat blootgestel was aan die inligtingstuk) aangedui het dat hulle positief voel oor die behandeling wat hulle gesplete-lip-enverhemeltebabas gaan ontvang. dit is egter moeilik om 'n persoon se houding deur middel van 'n enkele vraag en antwoord te evalueer (mcwilliams et al. 1984). die proefpersone se positiewe houding kan aan veel meer as bloot die inligtingsessie toegeskiyf word, byvoorbeeld persoonlikheidsverskille tussen die proefpersone in die onderskeie groepe. die twee groepe word verder met mekaar vergelyk in terme van die proefpersone se bewustheid van die hulp en behandeling wat beskikbaar is vir hul gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas. soos deur middel van statistiese berekeninge bevestig, is daar 'n beduidende verskil tussen die proefpersone van groep 1 en 2 se kennis aangaande die volgende vier aspekte: chirurgiese herstel van die spleet, ortodontiese behandeling en voedingsplaatjies, mediese behaudeling vir middeloorprobleme en voedingsen spraakterapie. daar was deurlopend 'n hoer mate van bewustheid van beskikbare behandeling vir hulle babas onder die proefpersone in groep 2 as wat die in groep 1 getoon het. nadat die baba se gesplete lip en verhemelte herstel is, bestaan die kind se behandeling grootliks uit spraaken taalterapie (van riper & emerick, 1984). deurdat die ouers egter reeds deur middel van vroee intervensie opgelei word om die kind se spraaken taalontwikkeling optimaal te stimuleer, word latere ontwikkelingsafwykings voorkom (bloom & lahey, 1978). dit is dus belangrik dat 100% van die ouers ingelig moet wees omtrent spraaken taalterapie en nie slegs 10% soos uit die vraelyste van groep 1 blyk nie. taalverskille tussen die swart ouers van 'n baba met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte en die oorwegend blanke span medici wat betrokke is by die behandeling van die baba, plaas deurgaans 'n beperking op die interaksie wat tussen hulle kan plaasvind. die feit dat spraakterapeute wat by hospitale en kliniek met swart pasiente werk, dikwels 'n swart assistent tot hul beskikking het, is dus 'n voordeel binne die vroee-intervensie-opset. die spraakterapeut behoort dus, met die hulp van 'n opgeleide tolk, soveel as moontlik inligting aan die ouers oor te dra en die kommunikasie tussen die ouers en die res van die multidissiplinere span te bevorder. nog 'n probleem wat direk binne die spraakterapeut se bemoeienis-area val, is die van middeloorpatologie. geen van die proefpersone in groep 1 was bewus van mediese behandeling vir middeloorprobleme nie, terwyl 30% van die proefpersone se babas reeds middeloorontsteking gehad het voor of ten tye van die uitvoer van die studie. 'n moontlike verklaring vir die ouers se oningeligtheid betreffende middeloorontsteking en die behandeling daarvan, mag wees dat hulle in so 'n mate met die baba se gesplete lip en/of verhemelte gemoeid was, dat hulle nie ag geslaan het op die baba se middeloorprobleme nie. middeloorprobleme het egter 'n hoe prevalensie by gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltepasiente (katz, 1987) en die ouers moet beslis tydens ouerleiding daarop attent gemaak corrieta campbell & brenda louw word, om sodoende die moontlikheid van 'n latere gehoorverlies en taalagterstand te beperk. gereelde besoeke aan gesondheidsklinieke kan byvoorbeeld aangemoedig word in ' n poging om voorkomend op te tree. ten spyte daarvan dat die proefpersone in groep 1 min of geen kennis gedra het omtrent die wetenskaplik gefundeerde behandeling wat hul babas gaan ontvang nie, het die navorsing wel getoon dat 100% van die moeders in groep 1 mediese behandeling bo tradisionele behandeling vir hul babas verkies het. dit is opsigself'n positiewe bevinding, veral gesien in die lig van dabuto-brown (1989) se bevinding dat die ouers van swart babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte in die noordelike dele van afrika, ten spyte van beskikbare mediese dienste, grootliks op tradisionele medisyne vertrou. dit is veral belangrik dat die ouers van 'n baba met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte bewus gemaak word van alle beskikbare hulpbronne en behandelingsmoontlikhede terwyl hulle nog binne bereik van die spraakterapeut en res van die multidissiplinere span is. die ouers wat ver van 'n hospitaal of kliniek woon, kan ook ten tye van die ouerleidingsessie aanbeveel word om terug te keer na die hospitaal, of 'n spraaken gehoorgemeenskaps-werkster vir verdere hulp en leiding te nader. laastens, beklemtoon macdonald (1978) die belang daarvan dat die ouers ook aktief moet deelneem aan die behandelingsproses. hulle moet deel he aan die beslissings rakende die behandeling van hul baba. dit is nie voldoende dat die ouers van 'n gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebaba bloot ingelig word oor die behandelingsproses en passief optree nie. hulle moet in dialoog met die multi-dissiplinere span verkeer en aktief betrokke wees by die hulpverlening aan hulle kind. die voorafgaande word as ideaal vir samewerking tussen ouers en professionele persone gestel. aanpassings as gevolg van kulturele verskille sal noodwendig gemaak moet word. gevolgtrekkings en aanbevelings effektiewe ouerleiding bestaan uit inligtingverskaffmg, opleiding en beraad (louw, 1988). ongeag wanneer die spraakterapeut se eerste ontmoeting met die ouers van 'n gespletelip-en-verhemeltebaba plaasvind, behoort dit 'n opvoedkundige of opleidingsessie te wees (massengill & phillips, 1975). deur inligting te verstrek aan hierdie ouers help die terapeut hulle om hul kind se geboorte-afwyking en die behandeling wat dit vereis beter te verstaan en ook om hulself makliker met die geboortedefek te versoen. dit is ook belangrik dat die ouers van die gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebaba emosionele steun van die res van die multi-dissiplinere span ontvang (starr, 1983). sodoende word die drie basiese beginsels van ouerleiding, naamlik inligtingverskaffmg, opleiding en beraad, bereik. huidige resultate dui daarop dat taalen kulturele probleme tot 'n groot mate oorkom kan word en dat effektiewe ouerleiding vir ouers van swart gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas in suid-afrika 'n werklikheid kan wees. hierdie populasie was tot op hede baie verwaarloos. die resultate van die uitvoering van die vraelys met die proefpersone in groep 1 het bewys dat hulle beslis behoefte het aan inligting, opleiding en beraad betreffende die hantering en behandeling van hul babas. hierdie bevindinge word ook wyd gesteun deur die literatuur, wat aan toon dat die ouers van gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas wereldwyd behoefte toon aan meer inligting oor, opleiding in en emosionele ondersteuningmet die hantering van hul babas. vanuit 'n kritiese oogpunt beskou, is die steekproef wat gebruik is vir die verkryging van die navorsingsresultate egter beperk, in vergelyking met die totale populasie waarop die gevolgtrekkings van toepassing is. tweedens kan die geldigthe south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ouerleiding by ouers van swart babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte 49 jjeid van 'n intragroepvergelyking moontlik aanvegbaar wees, siende dat hierdie navorsingsontwerp minder veranderlikes binne die toetspopulasie sou meebring, indien die inligting oor 'n langer tydperk versamel sou word (smit, 1983). dit blyk egter uit die resultate van die studie dat al die proefpersone in groep 2 wel baat gevind het by die inligting wat aan hulle verskaf is. die inligtingverskaffing het plaasgevind met behulp van die inligtingstuk wat saamgestel is op grond van die proefpersone in groep 1 se behoeftes. die opgestelde inligtingstuk blyk dus 'n bruikbare instrument te wees om die swart ouers van gesplete-lip-en-verhemeltebabas in suidafrika van inligting oor hul baba se kongenitale gesigsdeformiteit te verskaf. opleiding en beraad is ook ingebou in die inligtingstuk ten einde meer effektiewe ouerleiding te bewerkstelligdie spraakterapeute by hospitale en klinieke kan die inligtingstuk aanwend as vertrekpunt vir 'n ouerleidingsprogram wat daarop gemik is om sowel die ouer as die kind op die lange duur te bevoordeel. uit die voorafgaande bespreking blyk dit dus duidelik dat die spraakterapeut in suid-afrika 'n verantwoordelikheid het betreffende verdere navorsing, die ontwikkeling en die implementering van hulpmiddels om die ouers van swart gespletelip-en-verhemeltebabas op te lei en te ondersteun. 'n vroee intervensiebenadering, kan met groot sukses binne hierdie teikenpopulasie toegepas word. * 'n kopie van die saamgestelde inligtingstuk is van die eerste outeur verkrygbaar. verwysings anderson. d., nelson, j. & fowler, s. (1978). developmental assessment schema. in w.h. northcott (red.), curriculum guide: hearing impaired children (0-3gears) and their parents. washington, d.c.: the alexander graham bell association for the deaf. bloom, l. & lahey, μ. (19 78). language development and language disorders. new york: john wiley. brookshire, b.l., lynch, j.i. & fox, d.r. (1980). a parent-child cleft palate curriculum. developing speech and language. tigard, oregon: c.c. publications. bzoch, k.r. (1989). communicative', disorders related to cleft lip and palate. 3de uitgawe boston: little brown. campbell, c. (1990). ouerldding by^swart babas met 'n gesplete lip en verhemelte. ongepubliseerde voorgraadse navorsingsprojek: universiteit van pretoria. | cervenka,j. (1984). african mask with cleft lip and palate: cleft palate journal, 21, 38-40. j cole, l.t. (1981). blacks with orofacial clefts: the state of the dilemma. asha, 22, 557-560. dabuto-brown, d.d. (1989). cranofacial clefts in a black african population. cleft palate journal, 26, 349-343. fox, d.r., lynch, j.i. & brookshire, b.l. (1978). selected developmental factors of cleft palate children between two and thirty-three months of age. cleft palate journal, 15, 239-245. katz, j. (1987). handbook of clinical audiologg. 2de uitgawe. baltimore: williams & wilkins. krogman, w.m. (1979). the cleft palate team in action. in h.k. cooper (red.), cleft palate and cleft lip: a team approach to clinical management and rehabilitation of the patient. philidelphia: saunders. louw, b. (1986). swart babas met gesplete-lip-en-verhemelte: 'n morftofunksionele studie. ongepubliseerde doktorale proefskrif: universiteit van pretoria. pretoria: universiteit van pretoria. louw, b. (1987). black south african cleft palate infants: a sociocultural perspective. the proceedings of the american cleft palate association 44th anniversarg meeting, texas maart 1987. louw, b. (1988). vroee intervensie by jong hoe risikokinders (0-3 jaar). communiphon, 285, 18-27. macdonald, s.k. (1978). perspectives concerning cleft lip and palate. massachusets:fprescription parents. massengill, r. & phillips, p.p. (1975). cleft palate and associated speech characteristics. nebraska: cliffs notes. mcwilliams, b.j., morris, h.l. & shelton, r.l. (1984). cleft palate speech. st. louis: mosby. olmstead, p., hardwick, h. & fortier, s. (1980). alaska cleft lip and palate series. alaska: alaska resource project. rampp, d.l. (1984). velopharengeal incompetencg. a practical guide for evaluation and management. texas: pro-ed. siegel, b. (1979). a racial comparison of cleft patients in a clinic population: associated anomalities and recurrence rates. cleft palate journal, 16, 193-197. slutsky, h. (1969). maternal reaction and adjustment to birth and care of cleft palate children. cleft palate journal. 6, 425-433. smit, g.j. (1983). navorsingsmetodes in die gedragswetenskappe. pretoria: van schaik. smoot, e.c., kucan, j.o., cope, j.s. & aase, j.m. (1988). the craniofacial team and the navajo patient. cleft palate journal, 25, 395402. starr, p. (1983). cleft lip and/or palate behavioral effects from infancy to adulthood. illinois: c.c. thomas. strauss, r.p. (1985). culture, rehabilitation and facial birth defects: internal case studies. cleft palate journal, 22, 56-61. taylor, o.l. jj986). treatment of communication disorders in culturallg and linguisticallg diverse populations. boston: little brown. toliver-weddington, g. (1990). cultural considerations in the treatment of craniofacial malformations in african americans. cleft palate journal, 27, 289-293. van riper, c. & emerick, l. (1984). speech correction: an introduction to speech pathology and audiologg. new jersey: prentice-hall. zar, j.h. (1984). biostatistical analgsis. 2de uitgawe, new jersey: prentice-hall. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 39, 1992 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) impedance measurements in the diagnosis of meniere's disease linda lloyd, b . a . ( s p . & h . t h e r a p y ) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) speech therapy department, groote schuur hospital cape town summar y meniere's disease is discussed in terms of diagnostic difficulties, histological findings and treatments pointing to the use of impedance techniques with this condition. ten subjects were given a questionnaire, regarding the symptoms experienced at the time of testing and impedance measures were conducted. acoustic impedance values were higher in 80% of the affected ears when compared to the unaffected ears. four of these values were more than double those in the unaffected ears and three were above normal limits. therefore, increased endolymphatic pressure appears to be reflected in the test results. it is felt that impedance techniques should be.included in the test battery for the diagnosis of meniere's disease. opsomming meniere se sindroom word behandel in terme van diagnostiese probleme, histologiese bevindings en behandeling en dui.op die gebruik van impedanstegnieke vir hierdie toestand. aan tien proefpersone is daar 'n vraelys uitgedeel aangaandedie simptome wat ervaar is tydens toetsing en 'n opname van impedansmetings is gemaak. akoestiese-impedans waardes was hoer in 80% van die aangetaste ore in vergelyking met die onaangetaste ore. vier van hierdie waardes was meer as dubbel vergelyk met die van die onaangetaste ore, en drie was hoer as die normale perke. derhalwe, blyk dit dat verhoogde endolimfatiese druk weerspieel word in hierdie toetsresultate. die skrywer is van mening dat impedansmeting-tegnieke in die toekoms, moontlik in die toetsbatterei vir diagnose van meniere se sindroom ingesluit sal word. the difficulties in diagnosing meniere's disease have been mentioned throughout the literature, b o t h from the point of view of the audiologist and that of the ear, nose and throat specialist. little research has apparently been conducted into the possible application of the recently developed impedance techniques in the diagnosis of this disease. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaans vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, deseber 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 linda lloyd meniere's disease meniere's disease, an inner ear condition, usually manifests itself as vertiginous "attacks" with nausea, accompanied by tinnitus and hearing loss. following the appearance of these symptoms, which may be severe, remission periods occur 2 2 . prosper meniere first described this disease in 1861 8 , 3 3 and considered the hearing loss as its cardinal symptom 3 2 . however today, there is confusion in the minds of many, as to the symptoms and pathological changes which should be ascribed to the disease, due mainly to the fluctuation of symptoms. meniere's disease is thus difficult to diagnose. 7,25,26,32 important factor in diagnosis appears to be that all symptoms vary simultaneously i.e. they appear to be directly related to one another, 3 2 , 3 3 but even this is in dispute 2 · 2 5 . while some authors consider the symptom triad necessary for diagnosis 6 , 2 6 others do not feel it is necessary 2 7 · 3 2 while yet a third group of authors have added the symptom of 'fullness in the ear' to those mentioned by meniere. 2 4 , 3 0 diagnosis is complicated by the fact that while the symptom triad may be present when the disease is fully developed18, any one of the symptoms may preceed the others by months or. years2. the term 'meniere's disease' has undoubtedly been applied to many other conditions, 3 2 a n d the writer feels that new diagnostic procedures should be investigated to assist in diagnosis. it has been hypothesised that the symptoms found in meniere's disease may be related to increases in edolymphatic pressure. recruitment and hearing loss are believed to result from hair cell distortion due to pressure increases in the scala media 3 2 . feldman 3 0 , 2 6 f e e l s that the symptom of fullness in the ear is the subjective impression of saccule dilation, while vertigo occurs when the pressure in the endolymph has built up to its height. the remaining symptom, tinnitus, has not yet been explained8. originally, endolymphatic hydrops (an accumulation of liquid in the endolymphatic spaces7) was felt to be an essential finding in meniere's disease 1 0 . this condition was found to have produced dilation of the saccule which occupied the whole vestibule and came into contact with the stapes footplate. dilation of the scala media was also found. however, some authors have had negative findings in this respect 3 2 . generally there is agreement that fluctuations in edolymphatic hydrops occur, linked to symptom f l u c t u a t i o n . 1 0 , 3 0 . many treatments, either medical or surgical, have been developed around the relief of increased endolymphatic pressure 7 , among these are attempts to control the formation and absorption of endolymph 21,31,30 o u e to symptom fluctuations, the success of these treatments is difficult to ascertain 7 , although generally, they have failed to give lasting relief 2 4 . the writer feels that pressure changes within the / endolymph may be detectable in the impedancei characteristics of the middel ear, via the oval window. impedance audiometry impedance audiometry was first introduced by metz, twenty five years ago. it is widely used in europe and the united states, as an important part of the battery of special audiometric t e s t s 1 4 . the techniques are used and designed to diagnose and distinguish between middle ear pathologies 4 . determination journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) impedance measurements in meniere's disease 51 of the ear canal volume, under various air pressure conditions, is the basis for all measures with the electroacoustic impedance balancing bridge28. sensori-neural pathologies, when tested using impedance techniques, appear as normal ears, providing the middle ear is normal. there has thus far been no real distinction between these, using impedance techniques, other than the detection of recruitment in the more severly impaired e a r s 1 2 . three basic impedance tests are generally used in order to gain a complete picture of the middle ear function:tympanometry — "the measurement of eardrum compliance" and mobility "under artificially induced air pressure changes in the ear canal" 1 6 . the measurement of the middle ear pressure is included in the test. acoustic impedance measurement the measurement of the amount of resistance to sound waves at the plane of the tympanic membrane5. acoustic reflex measurements — detection of impedance changes as a result of contraction of one or both middle ear muscles5. the absence of this reflex may have several pathological causes9·1 2 while jerger notes that a small percentage of normal ears do not exhibit this reflex12. the acoustic reflex is defined as "that level above the threshold of hearing, at which a sound is just capable of eliciting a reflex contraction of the stapedius muscle" 1 6 , this is usually at between 70 loodb sl 1 2 . the reflex test is considered by j e r g e r 1 2 to be the most objective test of recruitment so far constructed. in interpreting the results of these tests, normative data should be known, however this is not yet available excepting in the case of gross middle ear anomalies13. in metz's study in 1945 wide variation in impedance values were found28 and jerger reports an overlap between normal and disordered ears 1 2 . there would be great advantages to be gained if impedance techniques could be added to the diagnostic test battery for meniere's disease to confirm impressions already gained by the clinician12. this is especially true in cases of mental retardation or gross physical handicaps where other tests would not be suitable. when considering surgery, which may be unnecessary or dangerous, confirmation of diagnosis is important 2 1 . variables such as stress, anxiety, fatigue (all exaggerated in sufferers of meniere's disease), and intelligence factors could be eliminated due to the nature of impedance t e s t s 8 , 1 6 > 2 4 . these tests have been successfully used in the diagnosis of psychogenic deafness, retro-cochlear pathologies and sensorineural hearing loss with recruitm e n t 9 , 1 2 · 1 9 · 2 9 which according to fowler1 6 is a sign of cochlear pathology and therefore to be expected in meniere's disease. in subjects with meniere's disease, jerger 1 2 found type a tympanograms with normal acoustic impedance. the acoustic reflex was noted to occur at normal intensity levels i.e. 70 loodb but at reduced sensation levels, indicating recruitment. however, zwislocki 3 5 , feldman 5and friedman 6 feel that impedance is influenced by the inner ear, notably the compliance of the cochlear windows and the acoustic impedance at the cochlear entrance 3 5 , which in turn is controlled by conditions existing in the inner ear 3 4 . pathology of the inner ear that may in turn effect the middle ear via the cochlear windows tydskrif van die suid-afrikanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkude, vol. 22, desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 linda lloyd may show some anomaly in impedance testing as suggested by macrae19. according to histological findings, meniere's disease would qualify as such a disease. method subjects ten subjects were obtained by contact with ent specialists 6 males between the ages of 27 and 68 years with a mean age of 50 years, and 4 females between the ages of 35 and 62 years with a mean of 48 years 3 months. due to difficulty in obtaining subjects, it was necessary to include subjects in whom presbycusis could have influenced test results. all subjects were white south africans from various income groups as noted by their addresses of residence. the subjects were examined by ent specialists within 10 months of taking part in the study and a diagnosis of unilateral meniere's disease was made, based on the symptom triad and case history data. otoscopic examination, no longer than 4 months prior to the study, confirmed that no middle ear pathologies existed. control for the experiment was supplied by the unaffected ear of each subject which was expected to show the normal impedance values for that individual. procedure subjects were seen individually and took part in the following: completion of a questionnaire pure tone audiometry (air and bone conduction) impedance audiometry (tympanometry, acoustic impedance test and acoustic reflex measurements) questionnaire the questionnaire was given to ascertain a possible correlation between the symptoms experienced and the test results. questions were asked as to which of four symptoms were present, in which ear, with a description of the symptom where appropriate. the date of the last attack was also noted. pure tone audiometry bilateral tests in the ascending order, for air and bone conduction were carried out, using a maico ma 24 audiometer, calibrated to the iso 1964 reference standard and situated in an acoustically treated booth (iac series 1604-act). bone conduction was carried out for the speech frequencies and narrowband masking was used where appropriate, in air and bone conduction. impedance audiometry the madsen zo 70 electro-acoustic impedance balancing bridge was used to obtain the following three measures bilaterally: a tympanogram with readings taken for every pressure change of 50 mm of water. middle ear pressure was also measured. acoustic impedance was measured in acoustic ohms and calculated according to the mathematical formula. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) impedance measurements in meniere'sdisease 53 thresholds for the acoustic reflex were obtained for 1000 hz, 2000 hz, 4000 hz, 500 hz, and 250 hz in that order, using the ascending method. auditory signals were provided by an amplivox 51 audiometer calibrated to the iso 1964 standard, the limits of the audiometer being as follows:250 hz — 75db 2000 hz 95db 500 hz — 85db 4 0 0 0 h z 9 5 d b 1000 hz 95db the reflex elicited was recorded for the stimulated ear. results and discussion q u e s t i o n n a i r e the symptoms included in the questionnaire were meniere's traid plus 'fullness in the ear' which is increasingly being thought of as symptomatic of meniere's disease 7 . 2 4 . 3 o , 3 3 although each subject had previously experienced all four symptoms, table i shows that at the time of testing, five felt free of symptoms and five reported symptoms. of these five subjects, all had tinnitus, four reported hearing losses, one had fullness in the ear and none experienced vertigo. the time that had elapsed since the last attack ranged from 1 to 11 months. symptoms experienced at the time of testing sub-subjects tinnitus hearing loss fullness in the ear vertigo none 1 x x 2 x x -3 x 4 x 5 x 6 x x 7 8 9 x x x x x 10 x total 5 4 1 0 5 table i. distribution of subjects according to number and type of symptoms experienced when tested. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkude, vol. 22, desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 linda lloyd it could be anticipated that not all symptoms would be experienced by each subject as symptoms are known to fluctuate,2·8 and only the active stage of the disease is characterised by v e r t i g o 2 6 · 3 2 . of the subjects later found to have hearing losses (seven in all), only four reported noticing such a loss. this may be due to compensation for the loss, by the good ear 2 3 · 3 2 for example in the case of subjects 5 and 7. tinnitus may also prove so disturbing that the hearing loss goes unnoticed 2 6 . increased endolymphatic pressure cannot be presumed because of the presence of a hearing loss and/or tinnitus as these are reported to become independent of pressure changes in the later stages of the disease 2 · 2 1 · 3 3 . only subject 8 experienced fullness in the ear which lindsay7 and f i c k 3 0 regard as an indication of increased endolymphatic pressure. it is felt that this subject could therefore show high acoustic impedance values. in the earlier stages of the disease, sataloff 2 6 r e p o r t s the occurrence of unpredictable remissions for a few days or years with an unpredictable number of symptoms remaining, although the later stages of the disease are characterised by the symptom t r i a d 8 · 2 6 . pure tone audiometry table ii shows that 7 subjects had hearing losses in the affected ear. bone conduction thresholds confirmed these losses to be sensori-neural. the only hearing losses seen in the unaffected ears were recorded for subjects 1 , 5 , 6 , and 8, all over 60 years of age who had falling sensori-neural losses. it is possible that presbycusic hearing losses may have been superimposed on the losses due to meniere's disease, thus giving the audiograms falling configurations wnich may not otherwise have occurred. sataloff states that with presbycusic losses, both ears will be affected at the same r a t e 2 6 . three shapes of audiograms were recorded for the effected ears:flat subject 5 rising subjects 2, 4 and 7 falling subjects 1, 6 and 8 controversy exists about expected audiometric configurations in meniere's disease. the report of the subcommittee on equilibrium and its management25 states that, in the early stages, audiograms may show flat or rising configurations. golding-wood8 feels that diagnosis may not be made unless the configuration is falling, as in the later stages of the disease. he also quotes wright (1942) and opheim and flottorp (1957) who found no typical audiogram for meniere's disease. ' journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) impedance measurements in meniere's disease 55 pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s 10 35 y rs bc ο ιη ο pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s 10 35 y rs cj oa ο ο ο pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s co ei a\ > υ < ο ο ιη ο ο ο ο pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s co os 00 >1 00 (j oa ο σ\ ο σ\ ο σ\ pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s co os 00 >1 00 (j < ο ο ο σ\ ο σ\ ιη σ\ ο ο i pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s to os t>1 "3· -a(j « ο ο ο <ν ο <ν pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s to os t>1 "3· -aυ < ο in ο ο ο γλ ιη <ν ο <ν οο pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s to os vo >-. πί v oa ο ιη ο ιη pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s to os vo >-. πί 1 ο 1 ο (j < ο τ}· in cr, ο ο tf ο ιη τί ιη τί pu re t on e a u d io m et ri c re su lt s in d ec ib el s fo r af fe ct ed e a rs s u b j e c t s os -a· 5» 00 -ah r(n h r(n > α χ 5 0 0 audwmeter at the^a ^ ^ s i a r l d a r d ^ti-wdrug'o ^ and after treatment, usmga i c > streptomycin very shghuy ο d r u g s were gender specific. • dddel, soos hoorbare opsommis g wmsmsms^-1 hierdie middels geslagspesifiek is nut. κ ϋ ϊ , ototoxicity c; introduction tuberculosis mycobactenû and here a g ^ hon cases of active ^ ^ ^ ^ ' r ^ e n d i n e l l i , 1988). lion new cases annually (fnedman ( t b ) i n redue to the rising incidence of tube ^ ω ^ cent years and the ^ c r e a s ng b a c t e m e n > g t e r . standard anti-tuberculosis ( a n b t b ) ^ g 'l 1993), l i n g > pablos-mendez. kilburn, c a u * e n ^ ^ ^ medical treatment has to rely on accompaof drugs, like the a m l n o g l y c o s d e j ^ ^ ^ ^ o t o nying adverse toxic reactions such as η ρ t o m y c i n toxicity, etc. these aminoglycos^e mclud p ^ ^ and kanamycin. streptomycin has a s e * c o m . the eighth cranial nerve, v e s t i b d a r d a * £ ^ ^ moner than auditory damage. kanamyc 1 9 g 9 ) toxic, causing mainly cochlear damage ( c o l ^ aminoglycosides l r cells in cristae or maculae of the ear. u n c & k a m e r e r > these receptors never regener^ . 0 f having 1985). this places the p h f l c l s v ^ c o u l d result from to weigh the possibility of morbid**^hat coul ^ aminoglycoside administrftion against the ρ effects of the infection being treated. ofthe organ ofcorti,that partofthecoch ^ jo detect the highest systematihear. this damagmg process y ( s c h u k n e c h t , cally progresses s e c t becomes visible 1974). by the time this dama^ng e " h z ω ^ on a conventional has been lost hz), valuable time for p " ™ ^ 1 ^ to the high freand permanent d a a g e has been ^ d b y quency region in the cochlea. * m q h z ) hearregularly monitoring the high j t r e a t e d w i t h ing sensitivity of j ^ v j a n > 1 9 8 4 ) . aminoglycoside drugs (jonndort ^ ^ a t aminoglycoside drugs are admin d e t e r m i n e d b y so-called "ototox^ally safe dobages to ^ ^ standard audiometryganging ftt frequencies hz, while the real and e a r i y ^ m a g ^ ^ ^ g e n e r a . higher than 8000 hz. w i t h t h e a d v e 2 q ^ uon audiometers ^ 6 5 ) and with very good test-rete^t re h e a r i n g a c u i t y of it has become possible to momw o f o t o t o x t h e s e i n d i v i d u a l s t o d e t e c t ^ v e r y e a r j ^ icity, long before j ^ ^ ^ g ^ ^ u v e actions taken to halt it has also been demon, , „ kdskrifvir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 43, 1996 die suidafrikaanse tydskrif vir λ r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 4 g.r. voogt & h.s. schoem. strated that cochlear toxicity is reversible in more than half of all cases, if detected early enough and appropriate steps taken (fee, 1980). recently many researchers have done further studies on high frequency hearing and thresholds, with the main aim of eliminating some of the differences that existed amongst the findings of previous researchers. okstad, laukli & mair (1988) compared high frequency air conduction (ac) and electric bone conduction (ebc) thresholds in adults; schechter, fausti, rappaport & frey (1986) and stelmachowicz, beauchaine, kalberer & jesteadt (1989) worked on age-related high frequency ac thresholds; frank (1990) examined high frequency ac thresholds in adults; and frank & dreisbach (1991) tested for repeatability of high frequency ac thresholds in adults. the vast range of differences between testing equipment, subjects and methodology resulted in considerable difficulties when an attempt at direct comparison of their results was made. there was, however, found to be reasonable agreement on high frequency ac thresholds and norms, but less on bone conduction (bc). therefore it would appear that many questions still need to be answered with regard to the exact stimulus pathways and precise components of the ebc sensation. with respect to these presently unanswered problem areas in ebc audiometry it would appear that the only truly reliable method of measuring the effect of ototoxic drugs on high frequency hearing using ebc, would be to compare each patient's post-treatment highest audible frequency test results with his own pre-treatment baseline results. as ototoxicity usually occurs bilaterally and given the fact that ebc cannot at the present time be effectively masked, the test results would indicate the bc hearing sensitivity of the "best" ear. this suits the purpose of this study as the only interest is determining the ototoxic effect of different anti-tb drug treatments on the highest frequency the subject can hear. methodology from patients having to undergo treatment for tb in a tb hospital, all 172 admitted to this hospital in one month were included in this study and followed up over a six month period. these included newly diagnosed tb patients receiving the standard tb medication (a standardised four drug combination of rifampicin, isoniazid, pyrazinamide and ethambutol), patients with resistant tb receiving kanamycin (15 mg/kg/day) and patients with resistant tb receiving streptomycin (15mg/kg/day). this group of patients consisted of 106 males and 66 females, ranging in age from 7 to 71 years old. from the test results of the original 172 subjects, 80 were excluded because they did not have measurable hearing above 8 khz, developed middle ear problems, did not show up for follow-up audiometry, absconded from the treatment regimen, had renal failure, their drug treatment was altered/stopped, or they were discharged from hospital. thus only data from 92 subjects was included in the analysis of the results. all of them had their normal hearing ( 7 8 khz ) and their high frequency hearing ( 8 8 khz ) tested twice in the week prior to commencement of any treatment, and thereafter once a month over a period of six months. the average thresholds of the first two of each type of audiogram of each patient were used as the baseline audiograms for each patient against which any later changes in hear ing was compared. as no effective masking was available for the high frequency tests, the test results indicated the "best ear" high frequency hearing for each patient. consequently, the results of standard audiometry of each p a . tient's left and right ears were also reworked to give a "best ear" test result, in order to be able to compare these results with those from the high frequency tests. a maico ma-41 audiometer was used for the standard audiometry and an audimax 500 for the high frequency tests. the audimax audiometer works on the principle of electrostimulation. the test signal is superimposed on a modulated carrier frequency and is delivered via mylarcoated electrodes into the skin over each mastoid. numerous studies have identified electrostimulation as a means of audio-transmission of electromechanical vibration in the bone and tissue structures surrounding the inner ear and the cochlea. it would therefore appear that the subject's bc hearing is being tested (sommers & von gierke, 1964). this audiometer tests frequencies from 200 hz right up to 20 khz, in 200 hz steps. the stimulus intensities can be adjusted from 0 to 120 electrostimulation hearing threshold levels (eshtl) in 1 eshtl step sizes. zero to 120 eshtl corresponds with zero to 60 db sound pressure level (spl) (voogt, 1987). even though the full frequency range audiograms were recorded every time for the high frequency tests, for the purpose of this study only the highest frequency that the subject was able to hear at the maximum stimulus intensity of the audimax audiometer, was taken into account. all test results for each patient were compared to their baseline test results to determine if any high frequency hearing loss (hfhl) had occurred. the hospital is built on a vast expanse of open field, resulting in very quiet surroundings. the hearing tests were performed in a large and unused dental examination room which is situated a considerable distance away from the main hospital buildings, resulting in an extremely quiet test environment. the treatment regimen for each subject was withheld from the audiologist until completion of the six month treatment period, and the information on the type of medication was given only as standard, streptomycin or kanamycin. therefore the test results were grouped into a kanamycin group (k-group), a streptomycin group (sgroup) and a standard anti-tb drug group (n-group). the test results of these groups were then statistically compared as were the results of males and females within each treatment group. results using standard audiometry, it would appear that none of the subjects in these treatment groups experienced any resultant loss of hearing, as no differences could be found between their final audiograms and their baseline audiograms. this, however, was not the case when considering their high frequency audiograms. in this case very clear resultant losses of hearing could be seen. therefore, only the high frequency audiometric data was statistically analysed. table 1 shows that the mean ages for the three groups were reasonably evenly matched, so the possible effect that age differences amongst the three groups could have had on the results, were negligible. table 2 reflects the characteristics of hfhl between the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) . . t v of aminoglycoside drugs in tuberculosis otot o x i c i t y o i * " the three « ^ ^ i t s e e n that in all three treatment from table 3 it c a n ° u s i g n i f 1 c a n t differences in averagehfhlbet n o t d e r specific, a m i n o g l y c o s ^ ^ ^ ^ w s w a s comprised of a comparison statistical analys g a n d n ) m r e s p t of of the three treatm nt g w a p i g ^ ^ d hfhl by the k x u e j ^ ^ ^ g r o u p s ( c h i highly significant mi p a i r w i s e comparisons m i s t showedthat the mean hfhl value b y the rank sum test sh f r o m ^ m e a n s m i n the k-group d i f f e r j g cimically the means the s-group a n d n p o u p ^ ^ ^ ^ ^ k . g r o u p i h o w . i n the sand n-groups dm & r e s u l t o f the mediever, suffered a marked tir cation they received. table l : age c h a r a c t e r i s t i c s ^ treatment discussion an unfortunate a ^ c t <,f o t o t o x i c i t y t o d ^ e f f e c t o r a y i t , „ f ττϊήι, b e t w e e n t h e three table 2: comparison of h t h l . deiw treatment groups. treatment group κ s ν η 23 12 57 age (yrs) mean 2,17 0,65 0,32 std. dev. 1,76 0,28 0,20 min. 0,20 0,20 0,00 max. 7,60 1,20 0,80 hearing tested regularly ̂ τ τ ^ ^ positive treatand femies were almosi t a b l e * c o m p a r i s o n o l hfffl· b e . » * » « λ » ϋ treatment groups»· lemaies w n u . » 1 hfhl (khz) treatment group male female k-group: η mean std. dev. 11 2,18 1,37 12 2,15 2,11 s-group: η mean std. dev. 5 0,68 0,23 7 0,63 0,34 n-group: η mean std. dev. 35 0,32 1 0,20 22 0,32 0,21 according to teale, uoioman « — whatsoever. it is also possible that this mil ^ r e d u c t i o n which reduction of 12,8 kjiz over * j linearly with ini s whether ^ ^ ^ t h e n . creasing age^ as t h e average ag ^ ^ & ^ ^ group was about 35 5 w h i c h a p . audible frequency of a b o u t 8 k h z b y g ^ pears to fit in well with the fact m a ^ a u d i showing a high frequency ^ ^ ^ e t al. (1989) 0metryfromthisageonwards_ s b d m a c t o w ^ b e g i n s at found that the loss m h * h * a , ( 1 9 8 6 ) 3 0 r s o l d · die suid-afrikaanse skrifvir kommunikasieafs, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 g.r. voogt & h.s. schoeman if this ageing factor is taken into consideration, then it can be deduced that in the s-group, the streptomycin treatment regimen actually had a fairly minor ototoxic effect, as the average hfhl of 0,65 khz should then be reduced by 0,32 khz, giving a 0,33 khz hfhl as a result of the streptomycin treatment. this may then also mean that the average high frequency hearing loss of 2,17 khz that occurred in the kgroup should be decreased by 0,32 khz to 1,85 khz, if the effect of ageing is to be taken into account. it therefore becomes clear that the kanamycin treatment regimen is more than three times more ototoxic than the streptomycin regimen (average hfhl of 2,17 khz vs. 0,65 khz). if the above-mentioned correction for the effect of ageing is taken into consideration, then it means that the kanamycin regimen is almost six times more ototoxic than the streptomycin regimen (average hfhl of 1,85 khz vs. 0,33 khz). conclusion this study showed that high frequency audiometry is superior to standard audiometry with regard to the early detection of ototoxicity and that the use of standard audiometry for detecting ototoxicity is really of no clinical value. furthermore it was found that the so-called "safe" levels presently used in anti-tb treatment still lead to ototoxic damage. these levels will most probably have to be revised. the kanamycin treatment regimen was found to be three times more ototoxic than that of streptomycin. ageing was also found to have a possible effect on the hfhl measured. if a correction was made for hfhl occurring as a result of ageing, then kanamycin was found to be almost six times more ototoxic than streptomycin. it was also found that kanamycin and streptomycin ototoxicity were not gender specific as males and females were equally affected. lastly it became quite clear that the specific effects of ageing on the high frequency hearing will require further long-term studies. references collins, t.f.b. (1989). tuberculosis understanding and managing the disease. south african national tuberculosis association. de vaal, j.b. (1994). aspects of aminoglycoside therapy. specialist medicine, xvi(10), 24-60. fee, w.e. (1980). aminoglycoside ototoxicity in the human. laryngoscope, 90(suppl 24), 1-18. fletcher, j.l. (1965). reliability of high frequency thresholds. j. aud. res., 5, 133-137. frank, t. (1990). high frequency hearing thresholds in young adults using a commercially available audiometer. ear hear, 11(8), 450-454. frank, t. & dreisbach, l.e. (1991). repeatability of high frequency thresholds. ear hear, 12(4), 294-295. frieden, t.r., sterling, j., pablos-mendez, α., kilburn, j.o., cauthen, g.m. & dooley, s.w. (1993). the emergence of drugresistant tuberculosis in new york city. n. engl. j. med., 328, 521-526. friedman, h. & bendinelli, m. (1988). preface in: m. bendinelli & h. friedman (eds.), mycobacterium tuberculosis. new york: plenum press. johnson, j.t. & kamerer, d.b. (1985). aminoglycoside ototoxicity: an update, with implications for all drug therapies. postgraduate medicine, 77(5), 131-138. okstad, s., laukli, e. & mair, i.w.s. (1988). high frequency audiometry: comparison of electric bone-conduction and airconduction thresholds. audiology, 27, 17-26. schechter, a.m., fausti, s.a., rappaport, b.z. & frey, r.h. (1986). age categorization of high-frequency auditory threshold data. j. acoust. soc. am., 79(3), 767-771. schuknecht, h.f. (1974). pathology of the ear. cambridge, massachusets: harvard university press. sommers, h.c. & von gierke, h.e. (1964). hearing sensations in electric fields. aerospace medicine., 35, 834-839. stelmachowicz, p.g., beauchaine, k.a., kalberer, a. & jesteadt, w. (1989). normative thresholds in the 8to 20-khz range as a function of age. j. acoust. soc. am., 86(4), 1384-1391. teale, c., goldman, j.m. & pearson, s.b. (1994). the association of age with the presentation and outcome of tuberculosis: a five year survey. j.ar.d., 6(4), 7-10. tbnndorf, j. & kurman, b. (1984). high frequency audiometry. annals of otology, rhinology and laryngology, 93, 576-582. voogt, g.r. (1987). early detection of ototoxicity by high frequency audiometry a case study. south african journal of communication disorders, 34, 67-70. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol. 43, 1996 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) november journal of the south african logopedic society pre-school language education for the brain-damaged child , ν r read at the 1956 summer meeting, alexander graham bell association for the deaf, june, 1956. a paper reaa j l b a n g s ) p h . d . it is essential that the speaker restrict his discussion in certain ways. in the first place, he does not have practical training nor expcnence in the academic education of the brain-damaged child and any comments made concerning this discipline would be based upon purely theoretical considerations. as a matter of fact the title of this paper should be "the language development of the preschool brain-damaged child," but in view of the over-all panel topic, the word education as inserted makes my presence more acceptable. there are so many facets to the generic term brain-damaged that we sometimes spend many professional years in happy argument with colleagues (who are obviously in error in their thinking) before we realize that we are not talking about the same child, the same age level, the same damage or even the same educational levels or processes. in view of this, i would like to define the various terms i shall be using. the children i am discussing are of preschool age and have central involvements which do not result in primary symptoms of athetosis, spasticity, ataxia or flaccidity. in other words, they are not cerebral palsied in the usual connotation of this term. it is, of course, true that these physically handicapped children may have some of the language problems with which i am concerned. it is possible that we do not all use the term language with the same concepts in mind. in this paper, language will refer to all of the symbolic processes which enter into human communication; it includes facial expression, emotional outcries, gestures and words whether they are perceived or used by the'children under discussion. the term speech, on the other hand, refers to the process of articulating words and though i am certain that speech problems of central etiology are commonly seen, i am not concerned with them in this paper. finally, the preschool children i wish to discuss are intellectually normal or dull normal, do not have significant sensory involvements of audition, and have given medical and/or psychometric evidence of brain-damage. though defective audition may play a considerable role among some of of these children, i am eliminating from discussion those with losses between 500 and 2000 cps in excess of 25 db. as well as those with precipitate losses above 1000 cps. it is not apparent that we are interested in, and working with, a rather homogenous group of children and i am sure that most of you have anticipated one of the most difficult problems confronting us; the procedures needed to identify these children adequately. proper evaluation is, in my estimation, the first step in the training process and, in many ways, one of the most difficult. inasmuch as we are interested in the brain-damaged, language-retarded child, the evaluative process usually starts in the home. parents become alarmed when their children fail to use words at appropriate age levels. their first stop is the pediatrician who then utilizes the services of the neurologist who in turn refers to the psychologist, audiologist and speech pathologist. this team provides the therapist with a delineation of the child's assets and liabilities, if the various members have done their work properly and, of course, providing each was able to conduct an examination. in addition, each member of the team conducts his habilitative-treatment function, though in many instances relying on the reports of other members when determining what to do. typing, duplicating, translating marie van der merwe's secretarial service (member of the s.a. society of translators and interpreters) 106 national mutual life ass. ass. building, cor. rissik & market streets, johannesburg. phones: office: 23-8084. evenings 46-1476 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society november language education i am concerned with the languageeducation process, so will devote a little time to a view of the team information as seen through the eyes of the teacher-therapist. from the medic, she wants to know the following: 1. does the neurological report show evidence of brain damage? 2. if drug-therapy is in progress, what is the contemplated effect on the child's behaviour, i.e. should it slow him down or speed him up? 3. are seizures to be expected or are they under drug control ? 4. if eeg results are positive are they focal or general and what interpretation does the psychologist make of the pattern? 5. if there is no apparent neurological evidence of brain damage, should the teacher observe the child for a period of time and report her observations before subsequent examinations are made? from the psychologist, the teacher-therapist wants to know: 1. the child's intellectual level. 2. his perceptual capacities — visual, auditory, tactual. 3. his social development levels, 4. if possible, some information regarding the parent-child relationships. the audiometric results should ultimately confirm the presence of hearing within normal limits and information from the speech pathologist should report the child's language level though in many instances the language evaluation is made after observation over a period of time by the therapist. after considering all of the information given her, the teacher is ready to begin language training and though training is a group process, each child's capacities are considered and each gets some individual work as he progresses. socialization is the first step in therapy. by socialization i do not mean just a reduction in the behavioral problems of the brain-damaged child. on the contrary, there are many specific processes to be altered and social behaviour changes accordingly. socialization in the sense i am using it involves alterations in the child's self-concept, development of parallel play, then cooperative play and establishment of identification with parents, teachers and other children. these changes are brought about by parent counselling, structured free play activities in which limits are gradually set up, and by providing each child with responsibilities commensurate with his age level. more specifically, these changes are effected by providing a therapy environment which minimizes previous frustrations such as a lack of oral communication, but provides recognition and builds up a sense of self-worthiness. parent counseling parent counselling is of paramount importance at this time. because of the child's language deviation, he is often overprotected and has usually been sheltered from his peers. he has seldom had the opportunity to play with a variety of children of his own age. the result is a dependent child whose parents are still undressing him, bathing him carrying him and catering to his every need, often even at the age of five years. paradoxically for the parents, they are usually confronted with a hyperactive, uninhibited and destructive child whose behaviour is so varied and unreasonable that it defies their understanding. parents are faced with a situation involving on the one hand their desire to overprotect, and on the other hand a rejection of their overtures by their offspring. the result is usually emotional confusion and ambivalence which is conveyed to the child. the teacher must also be able to accept the activities of her charges and not allow anger, frustration or impatience to appear. she must recognize that the same organic limitations which have caused the delay in language development may be responsible to some extent for the typical behaviour and inability to relate to others. by building a play environment in which the children feel accepted yet within which they are, to some extent, restricted according to the limitations set up by the therapist, speech therapists fully qualified are invited to apply for posts at the united cerebral palsy association of south africa, diagnostic clinic and treatment centre, situated at townsview, johannesburg. telephone 23-7034, or write secretary, box 10398, advising availability. part or full-time posts offered r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society the latter has actually provided her pupils with security. they then begin to identify themselves with the teacher, parents and peers. one of the important language by-products often seen in this period of training is the ability to express emotions appropriately. many of these children do not smile, laugh, cry or appear sad when confronted with situations appropriate to the expression of the emotion. part of the therapy programme, then, should be devoted to the development of situations which are funny or surprising to the group, with the therapist setting the example by an appropriate response. language stimulation the second step in training involves language stimulation. the children are given more play situations structured by the therapist in such a manner that communication among the children becomes increasingly important. the teacher, however, must g u a r d "against formal speech training during this phase. parents, siblings and relatives have constantly pressured these children to "say something," with a resulting frustration and finally rejection of speech by the child. de-emphasis on the act of speaking, accompanied by play situations normally requiring spontaneous communication, is imperative. at the same time the therapist must use simple language in abundance as she participates in the activities or develops the teaching situations. tape recording a complete therapy session and then listening to it will be helpful to teachers in this part of a training programme. it is quite difficult to confine your oral activity to stimulation and example and not to try to force a speech response from a pupil. listening to their part of the play session will reveal to teachers many of their errors. the language stimulation aspect of training, through properly directed co-operative and competitive play, will gradually bring about the child's awareness of a need for communication. at this point gestures and pulling may turn into jargon. an occasional word used by the therapist, or one of the children in the; group, will be repeated, possibly out of context with the actual play situation. again, no attempt is made to force a repetition or to emphasize speech per se. parent conferences on an individual and group basis should include discussions of the reasons for eliminating speech pressure, the development of a home stimulation programme that involves looking at and talking about familiar objects in the home or in suitable picture books, and considerable repetitive language during meals and other home group activities. the practice of showing off the child's new word to friends and relatives must be avoided. the next progression in therapy is concerned with the elaboration of vocabulary and useful speech. at this time field trips to zoos, farms, and other stimulating areas should be planned. the play activity within the classroom situation should be widely varied to provide for greater language example and stimulation. during this period useful words appear and become integrated into two word sentences. for the children language becomes a means of asserting themselves. the words which appear will be misarticulated more often than not, but no correction should be made. it is sometimes difficult at this time to keep from initiating speech therapy for articulation errors; however, the teacher will resist if she remembers that chronologically her charges may be four or five years of age, but so far as language development is concerned they now range between 16 and 20 months. from here on vocabulary development is accompanied by a school readiness programme. we frequently utilize the services of a good nursery school or kindergarten at this time by placing some of our children among normal speaking children for short periods of time each day. finally, they are left in the new school environment, but with a speech therapist observing and conferring with the new teacher when it seems essential. attention to articulation may accompany this phase of the programme. the programme i have outlined functions on several premises: 1—that these children by age four are in a language readiness state; 2—that language pressures and frustrations have added a strong psychological overlay to what was basically an organic problem; 3—that direct speech therapy or training will tend to further inhibit speech and language as well as to increase emotional problems; and 4—that the emergence of language follows socialization and development of a need for communication. dr. bangs is director of the houston speech and h e a r i n g centre, houston, texas. h i s paper was read as part of a panel discussion on " t h e education of aphasic childr e n " at the afternoon session, june 21, 1956. the volta bureau, reprinted with permission. miss bessie dembo 54 wingate mansions cor. smit & nugget streets, hospital hill, johannesburg phone 44-0860 typing and roneoing undertaken especially theses and students notes r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 editorial e t pierre de villiers pienaar it is with great and singular pleasure that the south african speech and hearing association dedicates this issue of the journal to pierre de villiers pienaar. professor pienaar, father of speech therapy and audiology in south africa, retired from the university of pretoria at the end of 1972 and is now professor emeritus of that university. those workers who have been associated with professor pienaar in the fields of speech pathology, audiology, phonetics and general linguistics are many indeed. the persons contributing to this issue were invited to do so in view of their associations with p. de v. pienaar. many more than are represented here have however had close links with him but the modest length of our journal prevents the publication of more papers than this issue is carrying. several who have had long standing contact with professor pienaar regret that they are unable to produce papers for this issue but at the same time express their very best wishes to him on the occasion of his retirement these are professors d.t. cole, c.m. doke, g. paul moore and c. van riper. the following contributors to this issue are honoured to be given the opportunity to pay tribute to pierre de villiers pienaar: professor aron, who covers the academic and professional life of p. de villiers pienaar, was a student in logopedics at the university of the witwatersrand and first worked as a clinician at the speech, voice and hearing clinic under his headship. professor westphal was a student in phonetics under professor pienaar at the university of the witwatersrand. dr. s.r. silverman of the central institute for the deaf, st. louis, u.s.a., professor r. luchsinger of zurich, and dr. p.h. damste of the university of utrecht, holland, have all had long professional contact with p. de villiers pienaar. margaret marks and isabella uys, senior lecturers, have both been students in logopedics under professor pienaar at the university of the witwatersrand and both worked with him at this university, mrs uys joining him later in the department of speech science, logopedics and audiology, university of pretoria. professor hay first joined professor pienaar at the university of pretoria working in the area of electro-acoustics and audiology, and on professor pienaar's retirement became head of the department of speech science, logopedics and audiology. professor l.w. lanham, head of the department of phonetics and general linguistics at the university of the witwatersrand, was a student in phonetics under professor pienaar, and dr william kerr, a distinguished oto-laryngologist in south africa has had professional liaison over many years in the area of voice disorders with p. de v. pienaar. we thank all those who contributed to this festschrift and join them in wishing pierre de villiers pienaar good health, and a contented, fruitful retirement. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vr spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y the cortical localisation of speech; 1. an analysis of preliminary difficulties m. wright. m.sc.. m.b.. b.ch. (formerly registrar at the national hospital for nervous diseases, queen square, london. at present electroencephalographer to tara hospital.) introduction. the initial premise of the discussion is that a lack of an accepted and relevant analysis of language, a s an aspect of human behaviour and experience, has necessarily caused a diversity in the classification, description and explanation of partial l a n g u a g e defects produced by localised cerebral d a m a g e . the formulation of the previous sentence reveals that a strong element of tautology may lie within it. hence advocates of any particular description of aphasic defects might agree that the premise essentially is a tautology, but assert that they possess the only relevant analysis of l a n g u a g e ; alternatively they could deny both the validity and the tautologous nature of the premise on the grounds that an accepted and adequate analysis of l a n g u a g e is not lacking (or perhaps is unattainable) and that diversity of terminology depends solely on disagreement concerning the physiological or psychological mechanisms whereby cerebral d a m a g e leads to partial failure of the l a n g u a g e process. the latter opinion seems untenable since the various terms applied to the aphasia reflect a divergence in the approach to l a n g u a g e behaviour rather than a disagreement on cerebral mechanisms ; "sensory" (aphasia) and "motor" (aphasia) presuppose an analysis of l a n g u a g e in the manner of clinical neurology, "expressive" and "receptive" a p h a s i a (weisenberg and macbride (10)) are a product of psychology, while "nominal" and "syntactic" are adjectives borrowed from the schoolroom study of grammar. such fundamental differences of approach preclude even a preliminary discussion of physiological mechanisms since the initial step in any scientific investigation, namely the formulation of a problem in relation to specific techniques, has a s yet been left undone. two examples (one hypothetical, another historical) derived from the neurological description of sensation illustrate and further emphasise the site of "pathology" in those clinical descriptions of language defects which presuppose on everyday familiar analysis of l a n g u a g e :— imagine the incredible difficulty which would result if the ascending fibre tracts in the spinal cord should have to be described without the analysis of sensation offered by the everyday words "pain," "sight" (vision), "touch" etc. the outcome could only be an anatomical description of nerve processes and pathological lesions, on which much useless philosophical comment could well be raised. however, confusion is averted b e c a u s e a n analysis of sensation does exist; moreover the analysis is both adequate and relevant since it is incorporated within every l a n g u a g e and thus can only be criticised a s being too subjective if it b e supposed that the whole of humanity is deluded. the second example concerns the deleterious effect on the physiology of sensation caused by the introduction of the two descriptive terms "epicritic" and "protopcrthic" sensation. walshe (9) has displayed the logical non sequiturs that are implied in the original definition of their terms and also the errors in clinical and physiological interpretations that may result from their use. the two terms are b a s e d on an evolutionary, and thus necessarily hypothetical, interpretation of certain clinical findings following nerve lesions ; they belong to abstract theory and not to observation nor convention ; they must stand or fall on an empirical test of their usefulness to neurophysiology and they are discarded because they are neither an adequate nor a relevant addition to the analysis of sensation. the argument that any particular classification of the ap hasia is b a s e d on an adequately instructed analysis of l a n g u a g e can only be refuted by showing that the analysis in question is not acceptable because it is not relevant. it is suggested that relevance in this c a s e can be equated with the notion "useful for physiological or experimental psychological investigations." thus an analysis of l a n g u a g e must at least contain terms definable by direct observation or by precise logical deduction from direct observation, and the formal statement of the analysis must have intrinsic logical r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y o c t o b e r cohesion; once the latter criteria are met, there remains the empirical test of whether there is enough useful correlation between the formal analysis and the processes of l a n g u a g e a s revealed in normal and aphasic subjects. the result of the empirical test determines the adequacy and relevance of the analysis. it follows from the two preceding paragraphs that no purely introspective or "mentalistic" description of l a n g u a g e is acceptable b e c a u s e it must lack definitions b a s e d upon impersonal observation and b e c a u s e it has not peculiar authority, possessed by the terms for subjective sensations, of incorporation within the everyday language of all people. the apparent insight wisdom and complexity of these theories represent a very real intellectual danger since, like all "supraterrestrial edifices," their castles in the air can be suitably moulded to fit successively the earthy environment of diverse and discrete observations. no less dangerous are theories which openly eschew "introspection" but forthwith rise to the even more rarefied atmosphere of "total cerebral function," "oragnism a s a whole" and "integrated personality." a second premise may now be stated a s follows; the analysis of the content, logical form and everyday use of l a n g u a g e belongs to the field of modern logic. it is probably true that many neurologists and speech therapists have never even considered the significance of the specific analysis of l a n g u a g e a s a separate field of investigation ; the term language behaviour may indeed seem unreal, unnecessary or even misleading. the clinical neurologist has approached speech via the spinal cord and brain-stem, thus, not unnaturally, talks of "motor" and "sensory" aphasia. unfortunately the terms "motor" and "sensory" possess but a fraction of their "spinal usefulness" for even the simplest problems of cortical physiology; they are necessarily even less effectual a s descriptive elements of the cortical mechanisms underlying languarge. modern electrophysiological research on the cerebral cortex has produced a terminology which is so specific to the experimental operations employed that it can have no application to the analysis of aphasia. moreover electrophysiology is an aspect of biophysics rather than clinical neurology and its techniques and terms are even less comprehensible to the clinician (who is not trained specifically a s a scientist nor a s a physiologist) than are the methods of logic. the speech therapist, on the other hand, approaches cerebral mechanisms from the more detached discipline of psychology (which assertion is not derived from first hand experience and is open to correction). the logopaedic approach, while more promising, would do well to avoid introspective analyses of l a n g u a g e and especially to avoid the presumption that clinical neurologists are in any way better equipped to analyse language in the normal or the abnormal; to which, surely, the medical literature on ap hasia is testimony enough. there are several relatively recent publications by competent logicians dealing especially with the analysis of l a n g u a g e processes. those of morris (6, 7, and 8), carnap (2, 3 and 4),bloomfield (1) and longer (5) are particularly relevant to the theme. within their works there is general agreement on the framework of the logical analysis of l a n g u a g e ; the scope and the precision of development of ideas show convincingly that this field of logic is not one into which an amateur may stray without much purposeful effort. to quote from bloomfield (1, p. 54-55):— "the subject matter of linguistics, of course, is human speech. other activities, such a s writing, which serve a s substitutes for speech, concern linguistics only in their semiotic aspect, a s representations of phonemes or speech-forms. since the meanings of speech cover everything (designcrta, including denotata ; syntactic relations; pragmatic slants), linguistics, even more than other branches of science, depends for its range and accuracy upon the success of science a s a whole. for the most part, our statements of meaning are makeshift. even if this were not the case, linguistics would still study forms first and then look into their meanings, since l a n g u a g e consists in the human response to the flow and variety of the world by simple sequences of a very few typical speech-sounds. linguistics is the chief contributor to semiotic. among the special branches of science, it intervenes between biology, on the one hand, and ethnology, sociology, and psychology, on the other : it stands between physical and cultural anthropology. l a n g u a g e establishes, by means of sound waves and on the basis of communal habit, an ever ready connection between the bodies of individuals—a connection between their nervous systems which enables each person to respond to the stimuli that act upon other persons. the division of labour, civilization, and culture arise from this interaction. popularly and even, to a large extent, academicr ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y ally, we are not accustomed to observing l a n g u a g e and its effects: these effects are generally explained instead by the postulation of "mental" factors. in the cosmos, langua g e produces human society, a structure more complex than the individual, related to him somewhat a s the many-celled organism is related to the single cell." evidently there has been no lack of effort on the part of logicians. therefore it seems to me significant that i can find no reference to such work in clinical descriptions of speech defects, and that in teaching neurology to logopaedic students there have been none who have heard of the logicians quoted above. at least an attempt at correlation would be interesting if not fruitful. formulation suitable for the empirical investigation the development of semiotic, the science of signs and languages, has been extended by morris (1938, 1946) in the vigorous manner of mathematical logic and also in the empirical tradition of objective psychology. in the following paragraph some of *he terms from semiotic are explained briefly, but there will b e no attempt to follow the precision characteristic of morris or carnap (1943). the description of l a n g u a g e processes (i.e., semiosis) may be divided into the related spheres of semantics, syntactics and pragmatics. semantics deals with any empirical observation or logical analysis concerning the relationships (i.e., the semantic relationships) existing between a linguistic expression and the object or event to which that expression refers; syntactics is concerned with the relations (i.e., syntactic relations) between two or more of the numerous symbols (generally words) within a l a n g u a g e ; pragmatics describes ι relationships (i.e., pragmatic relation) between linguistic expressions and the overt behaviour of such individual(s) who may utter or respond to the expressions. it is submitted that these notions constitute a preliminary analysis of language which does not outrage common sense and which presumably satisfies the logical criteria for a potentially "useful" analysis since it is a produce of competent logicians. it gives power, within limits, of abstracting three groups of entities (i.e., "objects of the physical world," "words," "human behaviour") and providing objective descriptions of relations existing within or between the groups. that a process of abstraction is involved is admitted, indeed it is axiomatic, but the abstractions are made deliberately and are well controlled; surely it is unreasonable to insist always that the process of l a n g u a g e "must be considered a s a whole" simply because it is impossible to do so. applying the three primary divisions of semiotic to descriptions of partial l a n g u a g e defects resulting from localised cerebral damage,it seems that any description in which specific reference can b e confined to the relations between words and objects to which they refer belongs to the field of descriptive semantics; any description in which specific reference can be confined to the relations between linguistic expressions (e.g., in the formulation of sentences) belongs to the sphere of descriptive syntactics, while descriptions in which specific and necessary reference must b e made to the patient's response to words (e.g., his understanding of words) belong to descriptive pragmatics. in any discussion of a partial l a n g u a g e defect, it is, therefore, important to decide whether reference to the patient (by name or personal pronoun) is incidental or whether it is a necessary part of the description. the' test situation should prompt the decision; if, for example, an object is shown and the patient names it, then the fact that the name is pronounced by the patient could be noted by several independent individuals and could b e described without reference to a particular patient; if, on the other hand, the patient's response to a written or spoken word is such that he apparently does not understand that world, then the defect can only be described by reference to the patient's behaviour. it is submitted that the common tests for ap hasi a and verbal agnosias can be classified into three groups which test the integrity of semantic, syntactic and pragmatic relationships respectively. further, the character of the test (the stimulus) and the observed response can be formulated so that an objective description may be given of any defect which is revealed. hence the terms semantic aphasia, syntactic aphasia and pragmatic aphasia are suggested on the grounds that they are readily and rather precisely defined from the test situation, and because they are derived from an adequate analysis of language. it then is necessary to decide if the analysis of language, and the terminology of l a n g u a g e defects derived from it, is not only adequate but also relevant. this is a matter for empirical investigation which must determine whether the proposed nomenclature "fits" defects actually encountered in clinical neurology. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d c s o c i e t y o c t o b e r elementary empirical testing this section must be the least complete within itself, for the field of possible amplification and application of terminology to clinical conditions is immense. one amplification will be introduced at once; each of the three primary types of application can be revealed by stimuli directed to the patient by one sensory channel (i.e., sight, hearing, touch) alone). it is thus possible to recognise visual, auditory and tactile varieties of semantic a p h a s i a ; visual and auditory subdivisions of syntactic aphasia, and visual and auditory varieties of pragmatic aphasia. the mode of subdivision is very similar to that used by nielsen (8) for types of agnosia and in the latter application has been proved very useful. a further amplification on the basis of the site of pathology will not be attempted since there is no s p a c e to present the evidence for cerebral localisation of the lesions producing each type of aphasia ; another amplification b a s e d on the type of "words" used in the test situation (i.e., "nouns" or "abstract words") suggests itself but i am not competent to apply the idea. the one application, which is chosen because it seems conclusive, is simply that types of a p h a s i a which would fall under the categories semantic, syntactic and pragmatic do occur in clinical neurology. semantic a p h a s i a s are described by nielsen (8) a s amnesic a p h a s i a s ; syntactic a ph a s ia is equivalent to the latter author's formulation aphasia, while pragmatic aph a s i a is called semantic aphasia by nielsen. the use of "semantic" by nielsen and his predecessors is indeed unfortunate and depended on the older and wider sense of the word semantic by which it included the whole of semiotic (i.e., it included semantics, syntactics and pragmatics). the case for renaming the older semantic aphasia, now calling it pragmatic aphasia, rests on the more authoritative claim of the modern analysis of l a n g u a g e and hence of "meaning." in my experience, the use of the proposed terminology gives a neater view of speech defects; it gives also further insight into the physiological mechanism of speech in the cerebral cortex, which insight is a powerful weapon upon the view that speech processes possess no precise cortical localisation; while finally it gives the most hopeful promise of clearing the "jargon" by which many descriptions of a p h a s i a induce a marked degree of pragmatic ap ha s i a among the audience. "pure" a p h a s i a s are very rare, but surely then, when they do occur, they should be studied by the best possible techniques among which the logical analysis of l a n g u a g e is certainly to be numbered. bibliography. 1. blomfield, l.—"linguistic aspects of science." international encyclopaedia of unified science, vol. 1, no. 4. chicago. university of c h i c a g o press, 1939. 2. c a r n a p , r.—"foundations of logic and mathematics.' international e n c y c l o p a e d i a of unified science, vol. 1, no. 3. chicago. university o! c h i c a g o press. 1939. 3. carnap, r.—"formalisation of logic.' c a m b r i d g e . harvard university press, 1943. 4. c a r n a p , r.—"meaning and necessity.' c h i c a g o : university of c h i c a g o press, 1947. 5. langer, s. k.—"philosophy in a new key." cambridge : harvard university press. 6. morris, c.—"foundations of the theory of s i g n s . " international encyclopedia of unified science. vol. 1, no. 2. c h i c a g o : university of c h i c a g o press, 1938. 7. morris, c.—"signs, l a n g u a g e and behaviour." new york : prentice-hal!, inc., 1946. 8. nielsen, j. m.—"agnosia, apraxia, a p h a s i a . " new york : paul b. hoeber, inc., 1946. 9. walshe, f. m. r.—"the anatomy and physiology of cutaneous sensation. a critical review." brain. vol. 6 5 : 48-112, 1942. 10. weisenberg, τ. h. and macbride.—"aphasia.' brattleboro : e. l. hildreth and co. inc., 1935. books on speech defects improving the child's speech : virgil a. anderson. price : 34/-. post 1/3 speech correction through story telling units : e. mcg. nemoy. price : 35/-. post 1/3. language and language disturbances : aphasic symptom complexes and their significance for medicine and theory of language. k. goldstein. price : 81/3. post 1/6. speech h a b i t a t i o n in cerebral palsy : marion t. cass. price : 25/6. post i / . cerebral palsy : j. f. pohl. price: 46/3. post i / . aphasia therapeutics : longerlch and bordeaux. price : 32/-. post i / . diagnostic manual in speech correction : wendell johnson et al. price: 23/3. post 1/3. speech improvement cards : bryngelson and glaspey. price: 55/6. post 1/6. stuttering : e. f. hahn. price : 25/6. post i / . vanguard b o o k s e l l e r s 23, joubert street, johannesburg telephone 23-3511 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y stuttering therapy an integration of speech therapy and psychotherapy. m. aron. the utilization of psychotherapy and speech therapy together is fast being recognised as an indispensable approach in the majority of the up-to-date speech clinics. the integration of these two procedures is seen most clearly in therapy with stutterers, and combined they have very reasonably the same objective in view. there are probably few today who still administer a purely mechanical and symptomatic approach to stuttering and on the other hand, few who ignore the symptom and are only concerned with psychotherapy. they are both necessary. the stutterer presents himself originally at the clinic as an individual with feelings of fear and embarrassment and he possesses a symptom which is regarded as being abnormal by his society. he is not presenting an isolated overt symptom—there are a chain of symptoms, affected by one another and having profound influence upon the internal and external aspects of behaviour. in the past it has been emphasized in many schools that speech therapy is dependent on other disciplines for its correct and stable maintenance, and that we must keep in mind the boundaries of our field. however, new techniques are continually being evolved where over-lapping into other fields cannot be avoided. progress in the fields of work dealing with people appear to be slowly amalgamating and inter-relating. if^we are going to look at a human being as a "gestalt" then perhaps we should look at the science dealing with him also as a "gestalt." that is not to say that we are eligible to practise in its entirety a science other than the one in which we are qualified; but we would be failing if we denied that we do use some necessary principles from other fields and that there is overlapping. the very nature of a speech disorder demands an organismic therapy. speech cannot be isolated from behaviour and personality. goldstein (1) comes to the following conclusion as to the nature of language — "language is a means of the individual to come to terms with the outer world and to realize himself," and eisenson (2) says "the function of speech is to cause response of thoughts, feelings or actions to occur in someone". language (speech) is a social institution and as such it cannot be removed from other aspects of our culture pertaining to ourselves and our thoughts without distortion to the picture as a whole. if we regard stuttering as avoidance behaviour (3, 5), and the stutterer as an individual on constant vigil so that he might avoid or postpone words and situations which are fearful to himself then we can expect an integrated approach of speech therapy and psychotherapy to be more fruitful. psychotherapy deals with the individual making new adjustments to life and finding more satisfying relationships where he is temporarly unable to do so for himself. sheehan (4) has said that stuttering not only expresses the nature of the stutterer s relationships but is in part determined by them. deep psychotherapy is of no concern to the speech therapist, but nevertheless the basic principles used, including mental hygiene, become incorporated in speech therapy. group therapy for stutterers practised in parts of the states and canada uses this combined approach. it deals with the handling of fear that the individual has generally and also the fears as regards his blocks, with the changes of attitudes towards himself, his speech and towards his particular societv group; as well as providing tools to enable him to appreciate the problem symptomatically and to change his pattern of stuttering. this approach is one practised by the "iowa school" (4, 5, 6, 7), and is the one used by douglas in toronto which i observed. because of the use of this particular approach in these groups, i.e., the integration of psychotherapy and speech therapy, it can be expected that problems and changes in the individual will continually occur. the material is dynamic, personal and individual and the therapy can be described as a process in itself. psychotherapy "is a procedure which is intended to make a progressive change in the patient" (8). also because of the elements constituting and causing language and speech, defects and the rehabilitation of these defects must also cause changes in the individual and in his total expressions, v r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y a r l if we regard a stutterer's progress as being due only to speech therapy in its strictest definition, then we are incorrect, for the changes that occur and make progress possible are due, by definition, to the psychotherapeutic and speech therapeutic aspects of therapy. this was clearly illustrated to me when i observed stuttering group therapy. the groups emphasized the part that attitudes and personalities play. while using a mechanical approach to stuttering, a group on its own accord (because of the very nature of a group) would, i feel, still make demands for psychological concepts to be made explicit as regards their problems. apart from the fact that group therapy is more expedient than individual therapy, it has many social and psychological facets. some of these are : (a) the group provides a ready "audiencereactor" and it can be considered as the first step in the stutterer's new social relationships. (b) it provides for the stutterer a number of people who, having worked together with him, can understand his problem more readily than the outsider, can emphasize, support and counsel. (c) the group can engage itself in feared activities and can enable the individual to do collectively what they otherwise might not have been able to do (9). (d) participation is made easier and more free than if an individual would have to attend meetings by himself. it is a more tolerable situation (10). (e) attitude and conflicts which previously the stutterer thought he alone was afflicted with, become rather common attitudes and conflicts that are exhibited by the group. three sudh groups were undertaken at the speech clinic at the toronto psychiatric hospital while i was employed there during 1955. douglas believes that the stutterer should be first educated in the problem of stuttering before dealing with the actual therapy. this should eliminate misunderstandings; clear up notions as to the cause, its nature and development; and indicate techniques and attitudes that will be used and sought after during the therapy process. the information helps to objectify his problem and will make clear to him the nature of his rehabilitation and make possible also for the intellectual appreciation of the problem. these groups were handled with three different approaches as regards the educational period and was extended where possible into the therapeutic situation. (a) the directive-diadactic approach. here the group was educated in the problem of stuttering as a group of speech students would be. this approach also carried over into most of the therapeutic sessions including personal problems. points were made and suggested but they were not pushed or stressed at all as regards the members acceptance or understanding of them. the least possible assistance was given in therapy unless it was made explicit by the members that they required assistance. (b) the socratic approach. this concept implies "that all knowledge lies within so that in reality education proper is a process of recollection or recognition. the truth so acquired is reached by a personal discovery." the word "educate" implying the same, derived from the latin, "educ", meaning "to lead out," "to draw forth," "to elicit." this approach was therefore non-directive, bearing similar principles to that used in "non-directive counselling." information was drawn out by asking the right questions at the right time. the members of the group disdiscussed problems in the order that they vacancy for speech therapist the eastern province cerebral palsy school, port elizabeth one speech therapist is required from the beginning of the first term 1957 applications should be addressed to: mrs. h. lurie, 30, wares road, port elizabeth. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a p r i l j o u r n a l o p t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y s desired and any point that was made in the group was made by the group itself. at times the clinician recapitulated in his own words what was being said. where possible this was carried over into therapy, but at times because of circumstances dealing with individual problems, it could not always be as non-directive as desired. the main responsibility was placed on the patient's shoulders and "support" was implied. he was encouraged to carry this responsibility rather than to lean on the clinician, (c) the directive explanatory approach. here the group was educated in the problem of stuttering most directively. they were taught as a group of children would be. each step was most carefully explained over and over again, simplified and illustrated. therapy was dealt with in the same manner and "laid on the line." (i.e., everything was handed to them on a platter). the three groups consisted of carefully selected stutterers. with the aid of tests, they were selected with regard to being intelligent and for being relatively free from the possibility of emotional breakdown (other factors were taken into consideration for selection). no attempt at all was made to establish controls or standards of any kind in these groups with a view to making later comparisons. although the approach to the three groups was different, the therapy for these groups remained, where possible, the same. that is, going into feared situations, practising nonavoidance; symptom-analysis (i.e., analyzing tricks of an avoidance, postponement, starting, release nature, etc.), changing the pattern, using prolongations, cancellations,. etc., besides the attention given to the personal problems (included here were those problems also arising because of specific speech assignments) of each stutterer. the third group was the least dynamic of the three. its members were prepared to accept the word of authority and laid all responsibility on this authority figure, hence cooperating little and avoiding tasks and assignments which required will and perseverence. the first two groups were, more dynamic. the material, presented was more versatile and mature in concept. more work was accomplished and to a great extent each member was aware of his own responsibility towards therapy. of the two, however, the second group (i.e., handled with the socratic approach) was by far the more fruitful, the more strenuous in terms of involvement, and the most interesting to observe. perhaps this was so because of the very nature of a more "non-directive" method. the members of the group were more personally threatened than would be otherwise. they were made to "discover" for themselves. with very little help from the clinician they made clear many pertinent facts about stuttering and discussed most of the basic problems that we are confronted with in the stuttering problem. they appeared to have an excellent understanding of the problem and related points raised in regard to themselves. the following are some comments made by some of the group members : in discussing various ways of measuring the severity of stuttering one patient offered the following—"measure the frequency of the stuttering and the distortion to communication —i.e., the ability to make sense." in discussing rationalization, one patient admitted, "i feel that stuttering is a kind of rationalization behaviour and i would feel bad if it were removed—it might account in our minds for our more basic inadequacies. perhaps we feel inadequate anyhow and that stuttered speech is not the only problem." in a general discussion of the definition of stuttering one patient said : "i feel it's a mental peter rothenberg (pty.) ltd. electrical and industrial instruments manufacturers of: instruments under the registered trade mark "prei." industrial and electro surgical instrument repairs 98, mooi street, johannesburg. p.o. box 2471 phones 23-0730 23-0888 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c o e a r l speech block—the block escapes from speech and the stutterer lets it bother him and tries to avoid it and that is the problem. as regards my own stuttering, the physical manifestations seem useless in that they don't achieve anything. but i still feel disturbed—i sleep badly and have a nervous stomach." another patient remarked in reply to this: i am interested in your stomach and bad sleeping—your physical weaknesses may result because of suppressing your feelings, so that the mechanisms you use are more socially acceptable; its a penalty to pay!" the stutterer in question replied that "stuttering outwardly would be difficult and i would rebel against it. for me the indirect way might be better. i have to get out of it at all costs." (this particular patient could be termed an "interiorized" stutterer as differentiated from the "exteriorized" stutterer as described by douglass and quarrington in 1952 (11). one patient said: "i see the great importance of fear and i can accept its concepts intellectually, but cannot do so on an emotional level. overcoming fear will overcome the problem altogether and i can see that it is not the problem of fluency that is so important —in fact attempts to maintain fluency by "hook or by crook" might worsen the condition by increasing the anxiety." another said: "i find that i cannot avoid using my old devices as they are too mucn intact, but i see that i ought to stutter when i don't feel like it, so that i can consciously bring these old devices out and replace them by new better habits. but at the moment i have not got the courage to go out and have spasms on purpose." these statements also serve to indicate the need for the group to think and discuss on the lines of personal problems and adjustments. they were neither encouraged nor discouraged to approach the problem in this way rather than in any other way. during the latter part of the group sessions questions were asked the members in such a way as to give an indication to the clinician, as well as to the members themselves, of their understanding of the problem. the type of questions asked also indicate the nature of the therapy. some of the many questions asked were: what do we mean by the objective attitude? if you were a secondary stutterer but became completely and absolutely objective about your stuttering, do you think you would still continue to stutter? deaf? maico & amplivox h e a r i n g a i d s a most versatile range of instruments, scientifically fitted to your individual needs by fully trained audiologists westdene p r o d u c t s ( p t y . ) l t d . phone 23-0314 box 7710 29 essanby house, (second floor) johannesburg a n d at the hearing centre, cape town what is a starting device and when does it occur? must you know the rationale behind each step in treatment? why? why is it that immediate pain and penalty and suffering seem to be necessary in successful stuttering therapy? why must you fail during therapy? would you rather stutter and not have fear or would you rather have fears but not stutter? assuming a person is not just lazy, what other reasons could there possibly be behind his apparent lack of motivation in doing the work that is necessary in treatment? many answers to these questions were quite disorientated, and confused, while others, even on the same answer sheet, appeared to be quite reasonable. this in itsell is an interesting point and one which i would be tempted to connect with the problem of resistance mentioned later in this article. considering the total results of these three groups, i would say that when everything is boiled down, the same problems remain and these are likely to come up with whatever approach is used ,but incorporating r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y the same therapy mentioned above). stuttering can be considered as a defect of and hindrance to human relationships and as such must be dealt with as regards this relationship. whether the educational period should be included or not, i feel, is a matter for debate. the problems that occur and present themselves to the clinician most frequently are those problems concerned with changes that will occur within an individual in groups sucn as these. we ask the stutterer to go into feared situations, to attempt to face up and not to avoid speech situations, to change hid pattern of stuttering, to solve old attitudes and find new and better attitudes and hence we must expect changes with progress. as the demands of therapy increase so does the anxiety. this seems quite unavoidable if there is going to be any growth at all. the anxiety manifests itself in many ways but the most common, i find, is in the form of resistance. this resistance refers to breaking down of old habitual behaviour. it is difficult to surrender old habits and attitudes. to the stutterer, as to anyone else, resistance is not new. he has been adept in it long before coming to the clinic and he wii use it in many deft ways to fool both clinician and himself. at times in a given situation, it almost appears that a patient might be stupid when it comes to clarifying a certain point and seeing the rationale behind it-something which had been repeated often enough to him and which he had previously appeared to understand. the most intelligent at times fail to comprehend a point when it has personal implications and even when they do comprehend, they cannot or will not relate it to themselves emotionally. yet this must also be seen in its correct perspective. it is a problem of therapy, but, i feel, it is a necessary problem and one to be regarded as constructive rather than interfering. resistance gives the patient the power to pull in the reins in his own time and to make the situation more tolerable, and when he can go further without great harm he will do so. in this respect we must attempt to understand his dilemma. sheehan (4) has said that "when a stutterer has rejected the opportunity of dealing with word and situation fears, he provides a focal point for the analysis and understanding of his own resistances." the breakdown and realization of these resistances by the patient, i feel, becomes an important issue in therapy and is a vital barrier to cross to readjustment in the individual and as regards his speech. besides resistance to anxiety-provoking situations this problem could also possibly be seen in the light of resistance to change itself. a further problem in therapy coupled with resistance is the stutterer's self-perception. it is surprising what little stutterers really know about how they stutter and what they look like when they do, although they might be aware of the moment of difficulty. i have seen some describe fairly keenly the stuttering pattern of another in the group, but when it comes to themselves, they stee.r sharply away from it, deny that they exhibit the particular behaviour that they do and are often confident that they are in fact disguising their stuttering to a large extent to the outside world. in this respect it can be seen that they will go a long way in attempting to avoid symptom-analysis and bringing up to consciousness the exact nature of their stuttering behaviour. successful stuttering therapy results in obvious emotional and intellectual changes, growth in maturity and sensitiveness, a new objective awareness in the stutterer of his own sensations, reactions, and feelings, as well as an increased awareness of the same factors in the people with whom he comes into contact. in short he has gained "insight" and perhaps this rather abstract ability is the crux to the whole matter of success. working for the objective attitude can be considered as being analogus to that of gaining insight. insight comes from experiencing and underr ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) β j o u r n a l o f τηεί s o u t h a f r i c a n ! l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y 1 a f ' r l l standing disruption, for without this unpleasant aspect, it woud not be significant. however, insight itself presents further problems to the clinician. what degree of insight should a patient have? how far should we go and how much can we, as clinicians, give? i think it would be reasonable to expect that in some patients a great deal of insight would be harmful and undesirable, particularly when the patient is not mature enough, or not emotionally prepared to face up to deeper problems. we should expect a patient to attain that amount of insight which is sufficient "for his use in the problem at hand. consideration must be given also as to the difference between "intellectual" insight and "emotional" insight, intellectual insight being ̂ where the patient' understands and can "see" the issue, but which does not effect a fundamental change emotionally. we see, therefore, that speech therapy can be related to psychotherapy in many ways when the above points, amongst many others, are taken into consideration, and i feel that therapists are very hesitant and reluctant to make this fact explicit. without attempting to use psychotherapy consciously, speech therapy, particularly when used with stutterers, nevertheless involves psychotherapy, and this fact, if studied carefully, cannot be denied. the dynamics of speech therapy in fact imply and make obvious a therapy based on human relationships. furthermore, theories of stuttering, preventative methods against stuttering developing, and many actual therapies, all amply indicate the significant part played by psychology and psychotherapy (3, 5,6,7,11). the issue that is suggested here revolves around the use and the implications of psychotherapy in the work of the speech therapist at the present time. i feel that it is not out miss bessie dembo 54 w i n g a t e mansions, c/r. smit and nugget streets, hospital hill, johannesburg. phone 44-0860 typing and roneoing undertaken, especially theses and students notes. of place to suggest that, at this stage, we as speech therapists ask ourselves—"what exactly do we do in speech therapy?"—to make quite clear the dynamics, criteria and principles we employ, in order to appreciate the scope of our work. references : 1. goldstein, k.—"language and language disturbances." grune and stratton, new york, 1948. 2. eisenson, j.—"the psychology of speech." appleton-century-crofts, inc., new york, 1938. "3. sheehan, j.—"an integration of psychotherapy and speech therapy through a conflict theory of stuttering." journal of speech disorders, 19 december, 1954. 4. sheehan, j.—"theory and treatment of stuttering as an approach—avoidance conflict." university of california, 1952. •5. van riper, c.—"speech correction : principles and methods." prentic hall, new york, 1954. 6. johnson, w.—"people in quandries." harper, new york, 1946. 7. bryngelson, h., chapman and hansen—"know yourself." burgess, minneapolis, 1950. "8. ingham, h. and love, l.—"the process of psychotherapy." mcgraw-hill 'book company, inc., 1954. 9. backus, o.—"the use of group structure in speech therapy." journal of speech disorders june, 1952. 10. bauman, s.—"an organismic approach to the therapy and practice of group therapy as a remedial technique in speech therapy." m. a. thesis, university of the wjtwatersrand, johannesburg. 11. douglass, e. and quarrington, b.—"the differentiation of lnteriorized and exteriorized secondary stuttering." journal of speech disorders, december, 1952. p.o. box 10482 telephones 22-4656 23-9432 abe etkind (pty.) ltd. wholesale merchants first floor, lincoln house. 57 pritchard street. johannesburg. world's finest hearing-aids. 3 transistor from £29/15/0 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sajcd cpd.indd vol 60 • december 2013 • sajcd 59 cpd december 2013 1. choose one correct answer: in patients with chronic kidney disease: a. distortion product otoacoustic emissions (dpoaes) detect cochlear damage prior to pure tone audiometry b. pure tone audiometry detects cochlear damage prior to dpoaes c. no signs of cochlear damage are evident throughout the course of the disease d. signs of cochlear damage are evident in the initial stage of the disease. 2. choose one correct answer: th e endolymph in the inner ear contains: a. high concentrations of potassium and sodium b. low concentrations of sodium and high concentrations of calcium c. low concentrations of sodium and calcium and high levels of potassium d. high concentrations of sodium and low levels of potassium and calcium. 3. choose one correct answer: cortically auditory-evoked potentials (caep) are believed to refl ect activity at the level of the: a. brainstem b. th alamus and higher auditory cortex c. cerebellum and frontal cortex d. peripheral auditory pathway. 4. choose one correct answer: th e p1 and n2 components of the caep typically have: a. long latencies in adults b. higher amplitudes in adults c. shorter latencies and higher amplitudes in infants d. longer latencies and higher amplitudes in infants. 5. choose one correct answer: in the study by almeqbel the p1 amplitude: a. changed signifi cantly when diff erent speech sounds were presented b. was similar for all three speech sounds presented c. was diff erent when comparing /t/ and /m/ only d. was diff erent when comparing /a/ and /g/. 6. choose one correct answer: th e early hearing detection and intervention guidelines by the hpcsa (2007) recommend that hearing screening: a. only be conducted on high-risk infants b. must take place before 3 months of age c. must take place before 4 months of age d. must take place before 8 months of age. 7. choose one correct answer: joubert and casoojee found that recording of hearing-screening results was done by: a. 27% of nurses b. 51% of nurses c. 80% of nurses d. all of the nurses. 8. choose one correct answer: based on joubert and casoojee’s fi ndings, infant hearing-screening results are recorded on: a. neither the road-to-health chart, nor the city of johannesburg blue card b. both the road-to-health chart and the city of johannesburg blue card equally c. patient-retained records only d. clinic-retained records mainly. 9. true (a) or false (b): th e lack of hearing-screening and associated record keeping can be ascribed to the high workload of nurses. 10. true (a) or false (b): subjective tinnitus can be caused by psychogenic disorders. 11. choose one correct answer: kanji and khosa-shangase found that the participants with temperomandibular disorder (tmd): a. more oft en reported tinnitus when compared to those without tmd b. less oft en reported tinnitus when compared to those without tmd c. more oft en reported the duration of their tinnitus to be constant when compared to those without tmd d. less oft en reported the duration of the tinnitus as constant in comparison to those without tmd. 12. choose one correct answer: th e study by erasmus et al. found that most of the respondents: a. th ought existing tools for written language assessment were age appropriate b. th ought that existing tools for written language assessments were suffi cient c. used formal tools to assess written language d. used informal tools to assess written language. 13. choose one correct answer: many studies in south africa found that english additional-language learners: a. oft en have a better understanding of mathematical concepts than their english fi rst-language peers b. display poorer phonological awareness skills than those of their english fi rst-language peers c. have lower self-esteem than their english fi rst-language peers d. none of the above. 14. choose one correct answer: speech-language therapists might be reluctant to manage reading and writing diffi culties in secondary schools because of: a. inadequate training b. lack of valid assessment material c. fear of change d. all of the above e. none of the above. cpd questionnaires must be completed online via www.cpdjournals.co.za. after submission you can check the answers and print your certificate. th e south african journal of communication disorders sajcd cpd a maximum of 5 ceus will be awarded per correctly completed test. 60 sajcd • vol 60 • december 2013 15. choose one correct answer: collaboration between speechlanguage therapists and the education system at district level should focus on: a. prevention and support for individual learners b. advocacy for the employment of speech-language therapists in schools c. the development and implementation of support programmes for teachers in areas of literacy and numeracy. d. policy development for prevention and support of learners with literacy and numeracy difficulties. 16. choose one correct answer: in order for speech-language therapists to strengthen their contribution in the education system, they need to: a. increase the number of learners receiving individual therapy b. build good relationships with teachers c. acquire a deeper understanding of the nature of learning, teaching and the educational process d. learn to speak an african language in order to assist more learners. 17. true (a) or false (b): inclusive education encourages speech-language therapists to use the ‘pull-out’ model for service delivery in schools. 18. true (a) or false (b): the use of animated picture stimuli vs. static pictures made no difference to the quality and quantity of grade 3 learners’ narratives. 19. choose one correct answer: the assessment of narratives can provide information about the child’s: a. comprehension of morphology b. social abilities c. attitude d. writing ability. 20. choose one correct answer: the language development survey was developed to identify: a. expressive language delay in adolescents b. receptive language delay in adolescents c. expressive language delay in toddlers d. receptive language delay in toddlers. 'ν elektromiografiese analise van die l1pfunksies van die gesplete lip-geval w i l m a u y s b.a. (log.) (pretoria) spraak-, stem en gehoorkliniek universiteit van pretoria opsomming 'n elektromiografiese analise van die lipfunksies van 'n groep gesplete lipgevalle, is vergelyk met die van 'n groep sonder so 'n strukturele afwyking. die toetsmateriaal het basiese bewegings van die lippe wat onderliggend is aan die bemeestering van labiale en labiodentale artikulasie, asook die uitvoering van sodanige artikulatoriese bewegings, beslaan. nadat syferwaardes aan die kwalitatiewe evaluasie (volgens die skaal van basmajian) geheg is, k o n rekenkundige gemiddeldes verkry word. hieruit blyk dit dat die eksperimentele groep minder spierspanning vertoon in die uitvoering van lipfunksies as die kontrole groep. omdat daar geen grense vir normaliteit bestaan nie, kan die mate van spanning egter nie as afwykend b e s k o u word nie. leidrade vir terapie kan uit die analise verkry word en o o k 'n m o o n t l i k e maatstaf vir objektiewe beoordeling van die geslaagdheid van die operasietegnieke. summary a comparison b e t w e e n the electro-myographic analysis of lip functions of speakers with a cleft lip and speakers w i t h a normal structure, is presented. lip m o v e m e n t s associated with labial and labiodental articulation were studied, as well as performance of these articulatory m o v e m e n t s . numeric values were awarded to qualitative evaluations (according to the scale of basmajian) in order t o c o m p u t e mean values. results indicate that, for the experimental group, less tension was present during the e x e c u t i o n of lip functions. as the range of normal tension has not yet been established, however this cannot be regarded as defective. certain suggestions for therapy can be drawn from this analysis, as well as a possible criterion forjudging the effectiveness of surgical techniques. / / die toestand van gesplete lip is baie afgeskeep in die spraakheelkunde, moontlik omdat ondersoekers meen dat die spraak van so 'n pasient baie selde beinvloed word. hierdie aanname word bloot gemaak op die akoestiese resultaat van die spreker, wat 'n subjektiewe evaluasie is, aangesien die mens as beoordelaar optree en spraak beoordeel in terme van dit wat hy hoor.3-7 by die gesplete lip is dit hoofsaaklik die m. orbicularis oris, maar ook bloedvaten senuweebaanverloop wat struktureel beinvloed is.5 deur operasies word die spleet herstel en daar word aanvaar dat daar dan funksioneel geen journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η elektromiografiese analise van die lipfunksies van die gesplete lip-geval 13 afwyking aanwesig sal wees nie. daar bestaan dus in die spraakheelkunde 'n versuim om vas te stel of die funksies van hierdie lip werklik normaal is objektief-wetenskaplik gemeet. is die operasie geslaagd? vind daar kompensatoriese werking plaas wat op die oog af nie waarneembaar is nie? kwalitatiewe afleidings behoort slegs gemaak te word, indien dit deur kwantitatiewe gegewens gestaaf kan word. die resultate verkry uit elektromiografie voldoen aan hierdie vereistes. om genoemde vrae te beantwoord, moet twee aspekte getoets word. primer moet die hoeveelheid spierspanning wat deur die aktiwiteit benodig word, bepaal word, en sekonder die senuwee-innervering wat aktiwiteit moontlik maak. elektromiografie is 'n ondersoekmetode wat uniek is in die opsig dat dit presies openbaar wat 'n spier doen op enige gegewe oomblik tydens enige beweging,1 omdat dit die elektriese potensiaal wat opgewek word tydens aktiwiteit, registreer.4 metode die doel van die studie was om die lipfunksies van die gesplete lip-geval te vergelyk met die van die normale en sodoende vas te stel: a) of die lipfunksies ooreenstem met die van die normale; b) of kompensatoriese werking ingetree het; c) of die operasietegniek geslaagd is; d) en hoe uit hierdie resultate, leidrade t.o.v. terapie verkry kan word. daar word gehipotetiseer dat die gesplete lip-geval minder spierspanning as die normale sal vertoon by die uitvoering van lipfunksies. die strukturele afwyking van die gesplete lip word as rede hiervoor aangevoor.5-6 proefpersone vier vroulike proefpersone is geselekteer deur die metode van afgepaarde vergelyking. twee proefpersone is 11 jaar oud (een kontroleen een eksperimentele proefpersoon) en twee is 19 jaar (een kontroleen een eksperimentele proefpersoon). die teenwoordiging van 'n gesplete lip het die eksperimentele groep van die kontrolegroep geskei. die volgende faktore is konstant gehou. taal a1 die proefpersone is afrikaanssprekend omdat daar 'n artikulasiebasisverskil tussen afrikaans en engels bestaan. gehoor suiwertoon-luggeleidingsoudiogramme toon aan dat al vier proefpersone normale gehoor het. intelligensie van die klasonderwyseres is verneem dat die 11-jarige proefpersone 'n bogemiddelde intelligensie het en die aanname is gemaak dat universiteitstudente 'n bogemiddelde intelligensie het. artikulasie die geoefende oordeel van finalejaar b.a. (log.)-studente is gebruik om die proefpersone se artikulasie te beoordeel. geen artikulasieafwyking is teenwoordig nie. tipe lipspleet 'n unilaterale spleet aan die linkerkant is by albei proefpersone van die eksperimentele groep teenwoordig en strek vanaf die inferior-grens van die bo-lip tot by die neusholte. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 4 wilma u y s operasies slegs een operasie ter herstelling van die gesplete lip is uitgevoer op elk van die proefpersone van die eksperimentele groep terwyl hulle nog babas was. op die een is 'n sekondere operasie uitgevoer om die volheid van die bo-lip te verbeter. hierdie operasie is reeds twee jaar gelede uitgevoer. (de palma et al het d.m.v. 'n elektromiografiese studie vasgestel dat die motoreenheid na so 'n operasie binne 21 rnaande regenereer en dat reinnervering weer plaasvind.)2 weens onvermydelike omstandighede kon die operasietegnieke wat gebruik is, nie vasgestel word nie. terapie proefpersone van die eksperimentele groep het nooit terapie ter verbetering van die lipfunksies ontvang nie. toetsmateriaal drie tipes toetsmateriaal is voorgeskryf. 1. basiese bewegings van die lippe wat onderliggend aan die bemeestering van labiale artikulasie is, bv., tuit en strek van die lippe en die trek van die mondhoeke afsonderlik na die onderskeie kante toe. 2. die vinnige sluiting en opening van die lippe deur die konsonant /p/ vir 5 sekondes lank so vinnig as moontlik te herhaal. dit dien as waarborg vir die bemeestering van labiale artikulasie. 3. 'n woordlys is opgestel wat bilabiale en labiodentale klanke in anlaut-, inlauten auslaut-posisies bevat het en al die vokale van afrikaans in inlautof auslaut-posisie. die woordlys is opgestel om die voorkomsfrekwensie van die klanke konstant te hou, koartikulasie sover as moontlik uit te skakel en om kort response aan te teken op die elektromiogramme sodat die evaluasie daarvan vergemaklik kon word. 'n disa-driekanaal elektromiograaf is gebruik met oppervlakte elektrodes wat uit silwer vervaardig is. die elektrodes het 'n deursnee van 2 mm. sodat dit lig is en gevolglik nie lipbewegings bemoeilik nie. elektrode-jellie is gebruik om elektriese kontak te verseker en die elektrodes is met kleefband op die vel vasgeheg. 'n stophorlosie is gebruik om die 5 sekondes van /p/-herhaling af te tel. die gronddraad is om die proefpersoon se arm gebind, en die twee elektrodes is eers op die bo-lip, aan weerskante van die filtrum geplaas en daarna op die onder-lip aan weerskante van die middellyn daarvan. met ander woorde, die. elektrodes is op die m. orbicularis oris geplaas wat na aan die oppervlakte gelee is en dus nie 'n naaldelektrode benodig nie. slegs die m. orbicularis oris word by die eksperiment betrek, omdat dit hierdie spier is wat struktureel beinvloed is deur 'n gesplete lip. by die analise van die elektromiogramme is die skaal van basmajian gebruik. hierdie skaal gee 'n kwalitatiewe interpretasie t.o.v. die hoeveelheid aktiwiteit aanwesig, nl., geen, waarneembaar, gering, gemiddeld, uitgesproke en baie uitgesproke.1 om statistiese verwerkings hieruit moontlik te maak, is syferwaardes vanaf 0 tot 5 in hierdie volgorde onderskeidelik aan die genoemde waardebepalings toegese. ', i al die toetsitems is geklassifiseer onder die batterye „gerond", „ontrond", „neutraal", „bilabiaal", „labiodentaal" en „rigtings", sodat dieselfde lipbewegings saam beoordeel sou word. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ,'n elektromiografiese analise van die lipfunksies van die gesplete lip-geval 15 resultate rekenkundige gemiddeldes is verkry en die volgende aspekte is vergelyk: proefpersone teenoor batterye. eksperimentele groep teenoor kontrolegroep. battery teenoor battery. proefpersoon teenoor proefpersoon. uit die syfergegewens blyk dit dat die kontrolegroep vir al die batterye meer aktiwiteit in die onder-lip as in die bo-lip vertoon in teenstelling met die eksperimentele groep. ook vir die batterye: „gerond", „labiodentaal" en „rigtings" vertoon die kontrolegroep meer aktiwiteit in die bo-lip as in die onder-lip in teenstelling met die eksperimentele groep. vir die ander drie batterye is die teenoorgestelde waar. die volgorde waarin die batterye geplaas kan word in terme van die hoeveelheid aktiwiteit benodig, is soos volg: a) „gerond", met die bo-lip die mees aktiewe; b) „labiodentaal", met die bo-lip die mees aktiewe; c) „ontrond", met die onder-lip die mees aktiewe; d) „bilabiaal", met die onder-lip die mees aktiewe; e) „neutraal", met die bo-lip die mees aktiewe; en f) „rigtings", met albei lippe ewe aktief, aangesien die spiere van die hoek van die mond die werking veroorsaak. bo-lip onder-lip gesamentlik kontrolegroep 3,24 4,07 3,66 eksperimentele groep 2,9 1,99 2,45 tab el i — rekenkundige gemiddeldes verkry van totale hoeveelheid spierspanning. hieruit kan dus afgelei word dat die kontrolegroep meer van die onder-lip as van die bo-lip gebruik maak, terwyl die eksperimentele groep meer van die bo-lip as van die onder-lip gebruik maak. uit die laaste kolom blyk dit dat die aanvanklike hipotese aanvaar kan word die eksperimentele groep vertoon minder spierspanning tydens lipfunksionering. afleidings ten opsigte van die operasietegnieke wat op die twee proefpersone gebruik is, blyk die operasietegniek van die jonger proefpersoon meer suksesvol te wees. die verskil tussen die gemiddelde hoeveelheid aktiwiteit van die lippe van die twee jonger proefpersone is minder as die verskil tussen die twee ouer proefpersone. die bo-lip van die eksperimentele groep gebruik skynbaar meer aktiwiteit om te kompenseer vir die strukturele afwyking. daar moet dus in terapie gepoog word om die bo-lip meer aktief te kry, sonder dat inspanning gebruik word. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19 desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 wilma uys die aandeel van die onder-lip in labiale artikulasie moet vergroot word sodat die verhouding tussen boen onder-lip nader aan die normale gebring word. daar kan egter nie beweer word dat hierdie spanning wat die gesplete lip-geval vertoon, afwykend is nie, aangesien daar geen grense vir normaliteit bestaan nie, en die kompensatoriese werking van die omliggende spiere nie bekend is nie. daar moet dus 'n studie gedoen word om al die spiere betrokke by artikulasie te oridersoek en norme vas te stel. slegs dan kan spierwerking as afwykend bestempel word en kan die mate van kompensatoriese werking van ander spiere bepaal word. elektromiografie leen hom ook tot studies waar koartikulasie bepaal moet word en dit kan met vrug hiervoor aangewend word. b i b l i o g r a f i e 1. basmajian, j.v. (1962): muscles alive. their functions revealed by electromyography. the williams and wilkens company, baltimore. 2. de palma, a.p., leavitt, l.a., hardy, s.b. (1958): electromyography in full thickness flaps rotated between upper and lower lips. plastic and reconstructive surgery, 21,448-452. 3. drillien, c.m., ingram., t.t.s., wilkenson, e.m. (1966): the causes and natural history of cleft lip and palate. e. and s. livingstone ltd., edinburgh. 4. fromkin, v.a. (1963): some phonetic specifications of linguistic units: an electromyographic investigation. working papers in phonetics. 3, 1-170. 5. pennisi, v.r., shadish, w.r., klabunde, e.h. (1969): orbicularis oris muscle in cleft lip repair. cleft palate journal, 6, 141-153. 6. slaughter, w.b., henry, j.w., berger, j.w., berger, j.c. (1960): changes in blood vessel patterns in bilateral cleft lip. plastic and reconstructive surgery, 26, 166-179. 7. spriestersbach, d.c., powers, g.r. (1959): articulation skills, velopharyngeal closure and oral breath pressure of children with cleft palates. j.s.h.r., 2,318-325. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the brain-damaged child and learning problems enid m . harrison teacher at the forest town school for cerebral palsied children, johannesburg. what are the broad principles necessary for learning and how does a child learn? for a child to learn there is a spontaneous process of integration and interpretation of all sensations, sensory, motor and proprioceptive, through all the experiences of living. the essentials of what has been learned are applied to and developed step by step in further experiences, resulting in the generally accepted stages of development in the child. for example, at the age of five years a child should be able to draw a square, and a diamond at the age of seven. these are learned tasks. as kephart says, learning is not a mechanical addition of performances but a true development. the potential and rate of learning, however, differs with each child. some learn more quickly than others in all subjects or perhaps only in a few. some learn more easily visually, others auditorally. some have better memories, others better powers of concentration, observation, etc. the child with a high i.q. learns more easily than the child with an average i.q., but a factor of great importance is 'drive' and how the child makes use of his intelligence. the slow learner takes longer than the average child to acquire the basic readiness skills which are a prerequisite to learning in the classroom. on entering school at the generally accepted age, the child is not ready for formal education and learning becomes a problem. if, however, special attention is given as early as possible to help develop 'readiness' the child may well be able, at a later date, to take his place in the class and learn with the other children. for the retarded child the process of integration and interpretation is slower as are the stages of development, and the final level of attainment is lower in all aspects of learning. there is an overall limited ability to extract the essentials of what has been learned through experiences and to apply this to other experiences. the child's mental age is lower than his chronological age. in the brain-damaged child, however, the i.q. (as measured by the s.a. individual scale, for instance) may be well above average, and yet there may be slower stages of development and a lower level of attainment in some aspects of learning. the parts of the brain which are damaged do not tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 12, nr. 1 september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 14 enid μ . harrison function adequately and so there may well be difficulties in the process of integration and interpretation. the i.q. tests show a scatter of attainment. for example a nine-year-old child may fail an item at the four-year level and yet succeed in several items at the twelve-year level. the word 'may' is a must when discussing brain-damaged children because no two are ever alike. some may have no learning problems, others may have one or two or many problems to a lesser or greater degree. in all children learning problems can be the result of emotional factors or physical defects, such as a hearing loss or poor or faulty eyesight. in order to illustrate clearly some of the neurological learning problems of the brain-damaged child, we will exclude from this discussion the above factors and we will furthermore discuss the child with an average or above average i.q. assessment the initial assessment of the brain-damaged child is undertaken by a team of workers—paediatrician, neurologist, ophthalmologist, orthopaedic surgeon, psychologist, speech therapist, occupational therapist, physiotherapist and social worker. hearing tests are essential with all athetoids and any other children when there is a language or speech problem, and in this field, as in all others, the younger the child when tested the better. many children, in the past, have been classified as aphasic when the real problem has been a hearing loss. this was before the finer audiometric tests were applied by our speech therapists. guided by the report from the 'team' the teacher's assessment begins and continues in her daily contact with the child. at forest town school, assessment and treatment begin with babies of a few months and home training programmes are planned by the therapists concerned. at the age of 2i-3 years the children are admitted to the nursery section and the assessment and training of the child 'as a whole' continues. when working with cerebral palsied children we know there is brain damage, but there are many children with no physical involvement at all who have minimal brain damage. they have the same learning problems as the cerebral palsied child. several children of this type have been taught in our school. in this article, the term 'brain-damaged' is used as distinct from the term 'brain-injured' which is often used to imply that the child has, in addition, gross behaviour problems due to damage of the brain. this type of child is also handled at our school. the assessment of the results of the i.q. tests of the .brain-damaged child is most important and of great interest. the psychologist uses several tests, both verbal and performance, as there can be considerable discrepancy between these spheres of ability. looking at the result on only one test can be extremely misleading as can be seen from the following case. a child of four years had an exceptionally high score on the verbal scale. in the nursery school and kindergarten she was not tested for perception problems and her poor motor performance was thought to be the result of a severe hand involvement. in the grades, the teacher suspected journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 12, no. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e brain-damaged child and learning problems 15 learning problems but the child's charm and verbal ability were such that she talked her way out of difficulties. she always had something of interest, not connected with the problem, to discuss. she also missed a great deal of schooling because of operations. eventually she was found to have space perception problems which have affected her work. although still excellent at languages, on a non-verbal test, the snijders-oomen test, she rates below average. as a teacher in this field for over twelve years one has learned through trial and error methods in the classroom, studying literature, and by working with and gaining knowledge from members of the 'team'. the work is not only rewarding but interesting, instructive and a constant challenge. the following cases are quoted to illustrate how inadequate development of certain basic readiness skills resulted in specific learning problems, and how, although the result in every case was an inability to learn to write, each child's lack of basic skills was different. this applies to all learning problems in brain-damaged children. also, failure to learn may be due to one, or a combination of factors. subject a, a child of six years old, with an above-average i.q., was unable to write. there was no physical hand involvement. she learned to read easily. visual and auditory perception was good and there appeared to be no other problems. she could copy simple drawings in plasticine but could not draw them correctly. she could draw straight or curved lines. she could see if her copy was not like the original and when working with plasticine would, with much trial and error, push the pieces into the correct positions. but when drawing or writing she did not know how to place the straight or curved lines in position. she wrote the letter κ like this this was ten years ago and there was no member of the staff who could help overcome the problem. then the idea occurred to me to see if moving her arm into the positions required for the lines in a drawing might help. i made her say the words describing the movement and position of her arm, as well as following the movement with her eyes. for example she put her arm straight out and said 'straight out'. then in attempting to draw a matchstick man she used this verbalization, associated with the position and movement, to draw arms straight out. it worked! we played at soldiers lying down, standing up with arms straight out, up, slanting and curved to the top, bottom etc. the children verbalized while carrying out the movements and interpreted, verbally, movements made by the other children. they gave orders to each other as to how to place their arms. remedial techniques tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 12, nr. 1 : september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 enid μ . harrison they followed their own and other childrens' arm movements with their eyes and 'felt' the position of the arms with their hands. pressure was exerted against their hands as they moved their arms into position to emphasize the feeling of the movement. matchstick, plasticine and gummed paper men were made and then drawn. so ά ' learnt to write by associating the position of the lines of a drawing with verbal directions, which were associated with arm positions, and then carrying out the associated movement. this was the beginning of what was called 'directional training' and this procedure has been used as a basis for much of my remedial work in developing learning readiness skills. ά ' is now in high school and doing very well, but of special interest is the fact that she has discontinued mathematics. in most cases where children have space perception difficulties, written number work seems to become a problem sooner or later. another child, who did not have nearly as high an i.q. as ά ' is in form iv and doing mathematics adequately. she had no space problems. subject 'b', a child with gross apraxia who could interpret drawings but could not carry out the motor movement, has learned to write through 'directional training'. subject 'c' could not copy a drawing either in plasticine or on paper. he could recognize pictures and drawings of squares, circles, etc., but could not analyse the contour lines of a drawing, for example a square, into meaningful parts. as a result fine discrimination was poor and so was reading. directional training not only helped 'c' to analyse drawings into lines straight out or curved to the top etc., so that he could write, but also enabled him to learn to read. though slow to begin with, he is now, at the age of twelve years, an excellent reader. subject 'd' could analyse, draw the parts and place them correctly but could not see their spatial relationship. for example, she wrote a κ like this subject έ ' could discriminate only if direction was excluded. to her, pictures of, for example, baskets with handles up or down or to the side were all the same and so were b's and d's, p's and g's, u's-and n's, etc. drawing, therefore, where there was direction was confused and she often worked from right to left. reading was, as a result, very poor and remained a problem for years. she had an above-average intelligence and her auditory work was excellent. subject 'f' could copy any drawing but could not recall it. verbalization, as used in directional training, was one of the methods which helped her to recall the 'picture'. for example she would say, 'b is a line straight down with a curve to the right at the bottom'. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 12, no. 1 : september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e brain-damaged child and learning problems 17 writing problems also include the inability to space letters and words, and to place letters correctly between lines. the disability here is not the failure to appreciate relationships within the whole, but the difficulty of relating one subject to another in space. the child, of course, must also have adequate eye-hand co-ordination in order to write. the cases quoted above illustrate how, in order to help the child overcome the lack of a basic readiness skill, experiences were made meaningful by using the child's abilities to reinforce the one which was inadequate. for example, the experience of positioning her body and arms was made meaningful for subject ά ' and was developed step by step in further experiences such as plasticine, gummed paper and drawing activities and finally writing. kephart, in his book, the slow learner in the classroom, shows very clearly the complicated processes by which all sensations are integrated and interpreted so that the child develops the basic readiness skills. the teacher must understand these processes so that she can lead the child through the missing stages of development. if the child has not achieved the 'readiness' on which learning in the classroom is based, the teacher must be able to find out why. the test used at our school, to find the 'why' of learning problems, was also developed through trial and error methods and is not standardized. it consists of a series of carefully graded activities in which the child's ability to recognize, analyse, synthesize, recall and follow instructions, visually and auditorally, are noted; as well as his approach to a task, his ability to concentrate and the length of the concentration span. in addition the test can be used as the basis of graded programmes of remedial work to develop basic readiness skills, from the nursery school upwards. training in learning for the young child begins in the nursery school, as well as training in socializing, forming of correct habits, language and motor development. through observation, the teacher begins to assess the child's potential and problems and, through play, helps the child to experience all sensations meaningfully. the kindergarten teacher continues to assess and help the child develop learning readiness. this training continues in the grades and it is then or later that new problems may become apparent. for example, the child in the kindergarten copies drawings but in the grades recall is necessary. very often a child in the grades may well develop a number concept and be able to write down simple sums, but later, when hundreds, tens and units are used, the child with space problems very often gets 'lost' in the positioning of figures on the page. the teachers in the standards must also be aware of the child's basic problems, even though these appear to have been overcome, as they may well appear in different and more advanced aspects of work at any stage. there must be close co-operation between all members of staff who handle a child and the case conferences, held regularly at our school, are invaluable because the child is discussed as a 'whole' from all angles. as with all children, the most important factor in teaching is for the tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 12, nr. 1 : september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 t h e editor teacher to make contact with the child, gain his confidence and inculcate a desire and love of 'learning' by helping the child to achieve success. the importance of the teacher understanding 'how a child learns' does not apply only to those who handle the slow learner, brain-damaged or retarded children. there are many children in normal schools who would be saved feelings of failure, frustration, fear and from becoming problem children if helped to achieve success in their first years at school. teaching, with a challenge, becomes really worth while! opsomming daar is indiwiduele verskille in die leerproses, selfs onder die sogenaamde normale kinders. die verskille word egter duideliker as ons te doen het met die afwykende kind. die verstandelik vertraagde kind sal stadiger vorder en nie so ver vorder as die normale kind nie. die breinbeseerde sal weer op sekere gebiede vinniger en op ander gebiede stadiger vorder a.g.v. die letsel. hulle mag probleme ten opsigte van die integrasieproses en interpretasie bied. om alle bykomende afwykings bv. gehoorverlies, uit te sluit, is spanwerk nodig. 'n volledige diagnose moet van elke pasient gemaak word. in die behandeling van 'n kind met persepsieprobleme kan gebruik gemaak word van taal en spraak om persepsie te verbeter bv. die verbalisasie van handbewegings in die uitvoering van 'n psigomotoriese aksie. die belang van basiese vaardighede wat die kind gereed maak vir die leerproses in die skool kan nie genoeg beklemtoon word nie. ook moet die kind in staat wees om te herken, analiseer, sintetiseer, herroep en instruksies te volg (visueel en oiiditief). die taak van die onderwyser wat werk met die breinbeseerde kind, is 'n uitdaging wat die moeite werd is. reference kephart, newell c. (i960): the slow learner in the classroom. columbus, o h i o : charles e. merrill books, inc. discussion of mrs. ε. m. harrison's paper t h e e d i t o r / ten years ago mrs. harrison, using a technique which she has termed directional training, experienced success when attempting to teach braininjured children to learn to write. it is interesting to examine this technique in the light of a recent postulate of the eminent russian psychologist and neuro-psychiatrist, professor luria. luria has long been concerned with the application of experimental method to the problems of behaviour, and rank's as an experimental psychologist of high repute. his interest in language is demonstrated by the importance which journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 12, no. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aphasia in children suggestions for management and training h e l m e r r. m y k l e b u s t * much has been accomplished during the past 25 years in the development of programmes for handicapped children. a major reason for these accomplishments is the cooperative efforts of parents, teachers, and other lay and professional groups. those who have benefited most from these combined efforts include the deaf, blind, cerebral palsied, mentally retarded and the defective in speech. there is now another group requiring such consideration and cooperative efforts, the group of children with aphasia. this discusion is an attempt to outline some of the needs of aphasic children and to suggest ways for meeting these needs, especially in the home and in the school. w h a t is language? before considering the problem of aphasia, it is necessary to analyze briefly the nature of language. aphasia is a language disorder and we cannot understand the aphasic child's handicap unless we have knowledge of what language means for the normal child. language is a set of symbols used by human beings to represent objects, feelings and ideas. these symbols might be auditory (spoken word) or visual (written word and gestures). when the word cat is spoken, the speaker is using a sound (verbal symbol) to represent the actual cat. the person hearing the word cat spoken must know that the sound which he is hearing represents an actual cat and not, for example, a dog. otherwise the process of communication breaks down. the same process and principle occurs in written language. words have meaning and are symbols for something. the normal child gradually learns that the spoken sound for cat means a cat; he learns the same for all other words in his vocabulary. he first has an experience, such as playing with a cat; then he learns to associate the word cat with the object cat. after making this association he can use the word for the object. consequently we say that language is a set of symbols which people use to represent objects, feelings and ideas. the aphasic child's problem is * dr myklebust is professor of language pathology in the school of speech, professor of psychology in the college of liberal arts, professor of neurology and psychiatry in the medical school, northwestern university, evanston, illinois. one of not being able to make such associations, or he makes them only partly and with much greater difficulty than does the normal child. before discussing aphasia further, a brief consideration is given to how normal children learn this complex set of symbols called language. l a n g u a g e d e v e l o p m e n t children are not born with language. one of the most important things they must do during the first two years of life is to learn a language. when we study the language which a child must learn, we divide it into three parts on the basis of the way in which the symbols (words) are used. language is used for thinking; that is, for talking to oneself. this is called inner language. it is used also for understanding what others say; that is, we must receive language from others. this is called receptive language. a third way in which language must be used is in making our ideas known to others; that is, we must use words to express ourselves and this is called expressive language. dividing language into these three types, inner, receptive and expressive, helps us to understand how the child learns language and also what is wrong when a child has an aphasia. children must first have experiences, such as learning to recognize what is said to them before they can leam to speak. the child first develops some awareness of happenings, the meaning of experience. this is the beginning of his inner language. it takes the average normal child about eight to nine months to gain this necessary experience and inner language before he enters the next stage and begins to use receptive language. in other words, after eight or nine months he begins to understand a few words which are spoken by others. it takes him from four to five months more before he enters the third stage and begins to use expressive language·, he is twelve to thirteen months of age before he speaks his first word. often we assume that the child's first spoken word is his first language. actually it seems that no child learns to speak unless he first has learned something about what his experiences mean (inner language) and secondly, he has learned to understand some of what is said to him. in more technical terms we say that language develops in three successive steps; inner language develops first, receptive language second and 6 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) expressive language third. this becomes of great importance in determining the type of aphasia in any particular child and also in giving him appropriate training. a few comments should be made concerning other difficulties which might occur to delay or interfere with the development of language. deafness interferes with the normal development of language, not because the child cannot associate the word with the object or experience, but because he cannot hear the spoken word. another important problem which interferes with language development in some children is fear and apprehension. if the language (words) they hear is largely for scolding, punishment and threats, they might become so arfaid that they act as though they cannot hear, or at least as though they cannot speak. all children, including the normal, deaf and aphasic, will leam language more easily if it gives them feelings of enjoyment and pleasure rather than feelings of fear and rejection. w h a t is aphasia? aphasia is a language disorder. the aphasic child cannot associate symbols (words) with his experiences. such difficulty varies from one aphasic child to another. however, most of these children fall mainly into two general types; those who cannot understand what other say and those who cannot speak. according to the types of language discussed above, this means that some have difficulty in developing receptive language while others have difficulty in developing expressive language. each of these will be considered separately because their problem and their needs are different. all aphasia is due to a disorder of the central nervous system. in medical terms it is referred to as a neurological disorder. increasingly in the fields of education, psychology and language pathology it is being referred to as a psychoneurological learning disorder. e x p r e s s i v e a p h a s i a the child who has expressive aphasia is unable to relate the words he hears to that part of the nervous system which is used in speaking. to use our familiar example; he hears and understands the word cat but because of deficiencies in certain nerve centres in the brain, he cannot say the word cat. expressive aphasia sometimes is mistaken for a simple speech disorder. while talking obviously is affected by expressive aphasia, it is not a speech problem as such. rather, it is; normally a difficulty with language symbols. the child with speech difficulties can say words even though they might be poorly articulated. the expressive aphasic's difficulty is to get words out; usually when he does say a word, it is well articulated. often these children cannot say any words until after three or four years of age. with appropriate training many have considerable speech by five or six years of age . at first when they are successful in saying words, it is as though they blurt them out unexpectedly; they surprise themselves and their parents. a moment after they have spoken a word, they cannot repeat it. this sometimes annoys both parents and therapists because they assume that if the child says the word once, he can say it again. actually this is not true. he cannot say the word again until the total circumstances are "just right". this might not occur for a few weeks or a few months because it is a combination of his activities, his interest and his nervous system. how to help him with these circumstances is discussed below under suggestions for training. the expressive aphasic rarely is confused with a deaf child because he understands what is said to him. occasionally he is confused with emotionally disturbed children because it is assumed that he can speak if he wants to. parents sometimes feel that he is stubborn and make strong demands on him to speak. such confusions are serious and add to the expressive aphasic's difficulties. r e c e p t i v e a p h a s i a the child who has receptive aphasia cannot understand spoken language; he hears the word cat but he cannot associate it with a real cat. he cannot interpret the spoken language of others; he cannot "receive" what they are saying so his difficulty is called receptive aphasia. it must be remembered that his problem is not due to deafness; he can hear but he cannot understand what he hears. in receptive aphasia the impairment is limited to being unable to understand speech sounds. other sounds, such as the cat's meow, the sound of the vacuum cleaner or the running of water, are understood. in rare instances it seems that a child might be able to hear but is unable to understand or to relate meaning to any sound. this condition is called auditory agnosia. in thinking about receptive aphasia, just as with expressive aphasia, it is a disorder of language. the receptive aphasic can hear speech but he cannot understand it because certain nerve centres in the brain, which have to do with the understanding of spoken language, are not working properly. parents usually become aware that there is something wrong in the receptive aphasic child when he is between two or three years of age; some parents notice something different and seek advice when the child is between eighteen months and two years. they often wonder whether he is deaf because the receptive june, 196 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aphasic responds to sounds inconsistently; at times he hears faint sounds and at other times he might appear to be quite hard of hearing. other characteristics of these children are described below. it should be emphasized here that receptive aphasic children can be confused especially with children who have deafness as well as with the emotionally disturbed or the mentally retarded. such confusions must be guarded against by teachers and specialists. progress is being made in the development of ways to distinguish between children with receptive aphasia and those with other types of handicaps. the receptive aphasic child is more affected and more disturbed than the child with expressive aphasia. we think this is mainly for two reasons. first, the receptive aphasic is disturbed in all types of language; inner, receptive and expressive. because he cannot understand what is said to him, he has difficulty in organizing and in straightening out his daily experiences; that is,, he is delayed in his development of inner language. in addition, when he cannot understand speech, he cannot learn to use it; he is delayed in talking. remember that being able to comprehend spoken language is necessary in order to be able to learn to speak in the normal manner. the receptive aphasic is disturbed in all language areas while the expressive aphasic is disturbed mainly in one area, expressive language. this is important when we try to understand the receptive aphasic. some might say that the deaf child cannot understand spoken language either, so he and the receptive aphasic are alike. this is not true because there is an exceedingly important difference between the child who cannot hear speech and the one who can hear it but cannot understand it. the chief reason for this difference is that the conditions of aphasia and of deafness are so different. deafness is due to deficiencies in the ear. while there might be similarities between deafness and receptive aphasia, it must be emphasized that the differences are most important. it is clear that the problems and needs of the receptive aphasic are widely different from those of the deaf child and therefore the training and home management of these two types of children also should be different. receptive aphasic children can be helped a great deal. usually as they gradually learn to understand speech, they learn to use it expressively; that is, as they learn what words mean, they begin to talk. they make their best progress when the parents and language therapists work together. parents can begin the home training programme as soon as they learn that their child is aphasic; special handling in the home is desirable immediately. language training by a therapist often is begun by two to three years of age. it seems that for the best results this training should not be delayed beyond the age of three. with appropriate language training and home management many of these children develop understanding of speech by the age of five or six years. in many instances they enter the regular public schools but frequently are in need of special help, at least during the early grades. characteristics of aphasic children in order to understand the aphasic child's needs more fully, it is helpful to analyze his behaviour. his actions tell us important things about his difficulties. the characteristics described below pertain mostly to children with receptive aphasia, but they also can be applied in some ways to those with expressive aphasia. the expressive have fewer of these characteristics than the receptives. i n a t t e n t i o n aphasic children are easily distracted. they cannot devote attention to books and play, or to other activities such as dressing and eating, as well as deaf and other children. they give their attention to anything that is before them, whether or not it is important to them at the time. they are very active and grab things a great deal. the reason for their poor attention is that their difficulty prevents them from understanding their daily experiences and from separating the important from the unimportant. they are not being bad. they cannot control themselves well because they cannot grasp the true meaning of their surroundings and of their experiences. an important aspect of helping the child in the home and in the school is to be aware of this part of his problem. p e r s e v e r a t i o n after the aphasic child begins an activity, he might have difficulty in stopping it. sometimes he begins running and will run until he is very tired and loses control of himself. at such times he might laugh and giggle compulsively; this sometimes is thought of by parents and teachers as simple silliness. actually, the child starts running and he cannot stop of his own volition, so he shows signs of overdoing it. this same difficulty is seen in the child by his not being able to wait as a normal child. if you give him a crayon and a piece of paper arid sit down with him as though you were going to show him what to draw, you will find that he cannot wait for you. he begins scribbling without being able to get the idea that he is to wait for you so that you can do it together. journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) this is shown in many other ways. suppose that you can get him to understand that when daddy comes home, you are going for a ride in the car. he might be pleased and happy for a few minutes; then he will become very excitable and want to go immediately. you say that you are not going until daddy comes, but this does not help. he becomes more and more disturbed and demanding. in some children this problem becomes severe. again, they are not being bad. rather, they cannot tolerate having things on their mind and waiting until the logical time to go ahead with them. we call this not being able to wait. only through patient management and training do they learn to keep an "idea" in its place until the right time to carry it out. h e a r i n g parents of receptive aphasic children often state that at times their child seems to be able to hear while at other times he does not. it is not that these children cannot hear. they can hear but they have trouble in listening. this is like the trouble they have in paying attention. to be able to listen means that they can pay attention to one sound, such as to what their parents are saying, and not be disturbed by other sounds around them. this is especially difficult for the receptive aphasic. while his parents are calling him, he might be giving his attention to the sound of a car in the distance and therefore ignore the call of his parents. hearing and listening are not the same. listening is the use of hearing. disturbances of listening are common in these children. o t h e r c h a r a c t e r i s t i c s as we work with these children, many other characteristics are becoming known. it is not possible to discuss them in detail here but several will be mentioned. aphasic children often are awkward and clumsy in walking and in using their hands; they hold a pencil or button their clothes awkwardly. they are not shy like deaf or normal children; they make little distinction between strangers and friends. this is referred to as a disturbance of social perception. they are slow in developing control of their toilet habitsj. this too is different from deaf and normal children. apparently it takes the aphasic child longer to learn than certain feelings inside of him mean that it is time to tell mother that he should go to the toilet. confusion, misunderstanding and bewilderment are common. we can appreciate why this is true ι when we remember that many aphasic children at first do not understand gestures well either; they must guess most of the time as to what is expected of them. sometimes they guess wrongly, as did the little girl who was helping her mother clean. the mother said that she should empty the waste basket in the box on the porch. she had quite a surprise when she saw her daughter emptying the garbage into the box instead. perhaps this discussion of a few of the characteristics of aphasic children will be helpful in clarifying their problem and in indicating how they need help both in the home and in the school. w h a t causes aphasia? aphasia can result from many different causes. only a few can be mentioned. causes are of three types: (1) diseases, such as meningitis during infancy or german measles in the mother during pregnancy, might damage brain tissues and cause aphasia. (2) injuries occuring during delivery might cause minor brain hemorrhages or anoxia (lack of oxygen) and result in aphasia. (3) a defect in the development of certain brain tissues also might occur and cause an aphasia. it is interesting that at present it seems that receptive aphasia develops most frequently from the first two types of causes given here and that expressive aphasia develops most frequently from the last type. suggestions for management and training there are many important ways in which, parents can help their child to develop language. a few suggestions for home training are given below. o v e r s t i m u l a t i o n one of the aphasic child's greatest difficulties is his inability to tolerate normal stimulation. when he is confronted with the typical rushing about — television, toys, dressing, eating, visitors, going to the store and many other happenings in the home — he becomes over-stimulated. he cannot integrate and understand all of these experiences so he becomes distracted, bewildered, confused. this is not because he is mentally retarded, but because his mind does not get all the experiences to get together and make sense as easily as do other children. therefore, he should not be exposed to happenings which are beyond his ability to handle. for example, many parents have said that their child becomes uncontrollable at the super market. just think of the tremendous stimulation he is receiving in such a store. there are thousands of objects and colours, many people, and perhaps the child has to ride in an unfamiliar grocery cart, etc. the average child enjoys this stimulation and experience while to the aphasic it can be overwhelming. other common experiences which often june, 1961 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) overstimulate these children, are going to a restuarant or being required to be present with guests in the home. there are many other such situations. it is necessary for the language therapist to help the parents learn to notice when their child is being overstimulated. when this occurs, it is wise to take him out of the situation and give him assurance in a calm, deliberate and patient manner. after being out of the happenings and being "protected" from them for a few minutes, often he is ready to come back to the activity. l i s t e n i n g receptive aphasic children especially are in need of help in learning how to give attention to sounds which are important. this is done by playing games which depend upon listening. toys which produce sounds, such as quacking duck or a growling bear, can be used. the parent and the child each should have the same kind of toy. to begin, you make the sound in full view of the child and he gradually learns to imitate it with his toy. the next step is for you to hold your toy behind your back and have the child imitate the sound. the third step is for you to hide and then make the sound; the child is to find you after he has listened for the sound. as he learns the game, he hides and you find him. the entire activity should be playful; not boisterous, demanding, or overstimulating. use one sound at a time. when the child is successful with one, go on to others. talk to him in single words while playing these games with him. the reason we do not begin by having the child listen to spoken words instead of to toy sounds is that usually he is not ready to listen for words until he has learned to listen for other (nonverbal) sounds. r o u t i n e s for e a t i n g the circumstances centred around eating, frequently are difficult for the aphasic child. he must be able to conform to the demands of the family and also to understand the varying routines for feeding himself. at times he is to use a spoon, then a fork; he is to understand which foods are eaten in what way. t o help him at such times, it is wise to have him seated next to the same person from meal to meal. with some children who are highly distractible, it is helpful to have them eat before or after the rest of the family so that the stimulation is less. some parents have found it helpful to give the child an appropriate plate, a spoon or a fork and put only one food on the plate at a time. this makes it easier for him than when he has his milk, several foods and a spoon, knife and fork before him from which to choose. t o i l e t t r a i n i n g aphasic children require more time than other children to learn toilet habits. usually they are not ready to begin such training until they have been helped with their distraction and until they have understanding of their daily experiences; that is, until inner language has begun to develop. when training is begun it should be done with patience and with consideration of the child's problem. this is difficult, as it was for the parents whose aphasic child was five years of age before he was trained successfully. we know that this child had good abilities so we assisted the parents with his problem for three years. he is now seven years of age and is doing well in public school. the training should consist of good timing, it should be consistent and it should be simple in demonstration. if no success is achieved in a few weeks, it is wise to stop all attempts for a week or two then start the training programme again. l a n g u a g e encourage language but do not demand speech. it is understandable why parents are greatly distressed when their child does not begin to talk at the expected age. however, demands that he say words for what he wants or any other demands that he talk, usually make the child's problem more difficult and may even further delay his beginning to talk. whenever he uses his voice for calling you or for indicating what he wants, you should accept it as though he were talking even though the sounds are nothing like real words. the sooner the child can use his voice for expressing ideas, no matter how simple, the sooner he will develop language and speech. for example, a child of three who had a severe aphasia, when shown a doll's dining room table, said "namnam". she was able to use a number of such sounds for daily experiences. when sounds like these are used, they should be understood by you and you might say. "yes, janie eat." encourage the child to try to understand and to express himself in any way that he can, including the use of gestures. such encouragement helps him to prepare for and to develop more normal language. p l a y most toys are made for children who have normal language and thus can use imagination and ideas in a normal way. many aphasic children get no enjoyment from a fire truck with a siren. they have not been able to make any association between a toy and the exciting experience of seeing the real fire truck pass. to make this association requires language and 1 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a degree of imagination which they do not have at the usual age. to help your child learn to play, use toys which represent daily life — a toy car for daddy's car, a toy chair and table for the one he uses, a toy baith tub, some small figures to represent members of the family, a toy dog for the dog next door, a toy bed for his bed etc. as he associates these toys with his daily experiences, he will gradually learn to use them in playing games, such as "daddy comes home." "johnnie goes to bed" and "mary helps mommy clean." from these games you can go on to more imaginative and abstract play. development of the ability to "pretend" is very important because it is the basis of inner language; the child will not understand or use speech until inner language has begun to appear. discipline use patient firmness, not punishment. the usual methods of discipline are ineffective with aphasic children. this is because of the way in which they differ from other children in their needs and understanding. the discipline in most homes is based on the child's being able to understand that what he has done is not what was expected of him and the punishment is to make him realize that he is not to do it again. you see that typical discipline assumes that the child makes associations and used ideas well. if this type of discipline is used with aphasic children, they might become more distracted, more excitable and more difficult to manage. many parents have found the method of "firmness, without anger" helpful in managing their child. this means that definite yes or not lines are used and adhered to consistently from day to day; no is said firmly but not in anger. as a matter of fact, we find that we can say no firmly and then smile and get good results. apparently anger adds to his feelings of bewilderment and confusion. he learns to abide by the no more easily when he is not confused or threatened by' anger. j language therapy language training by professionally trained therapists is necessary for the best welfare of the aphasic child. more therapists trained especially in the techniques of language development are needed to meet this need. a few of the principles and proceedures which have been found beneficial will be described briefly. most of these suggestions assume that the therapist is working with only a few children at a time. some of the suggestions given above, in addition to those given below, are considered applicable to classroom situations after the child has entered a regular school. the training room the most suitable training room is one which is small, highly uncluttered and simply decorated. toys, materials and figures on the wall may serve as distracting influences. control proximity if the child is near you, he attends longer and controls his distraction. only after training can he engage himself successfully in the distance. your presence helps him assimilate experience and to integrate it meaningfully. gradually you can increase the distance between yourself and the child. the use of materials the use of concrete objejcts is desirable as compared to the use of pictorial materials. these objects should represent the child's daily life; they have the added advantage of permitting him to handle and to feel them. this simplifies the perceptual task. when pictures are used they should be simple and easily distinguished from the background. many children's books present a difficult perceptual task because the figure is presented against a field, a cloud, a sunset, etc. a strongly outlined drawing with a plain background is more useful, especially in early training. the use of concrete objects has another distinct advantage in that the child can manipulate them when expressing ideas. the language development approach the teacher and therapist should be aware of the child's total needs and pursue language training onl) at the level of his tolerance for stimulation and success. the training should be based on the child's major language problem. children having receptive aphasia usually require assistance mainly with inner and receptive language, whereas those having expressive aphasia need training only with expressive language. in all such language training the therapist should be aware that although articulation involvements might be present, the basic disorder is not a speech defect. correction of articulation should be deferred until language usage has met practical communication needs in daily life. a method which has proved useful for the development of language will be outlined briefly. for maximum results with this procedure, it is necessary to acquire some knowledge of the psychology of language, language pathology, and of educational methods used with young children, as well as training june, 1961 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) and experience in the area of psychoneurology. the references given below are sources which provide an opportunity for background reading. the basic principle on which this method is based includes the assumption that the child must first have a meaningful experience and then relate or associate the symbol with it. therefore, in an organized manner, determined by the child's performance level, he is engaged in an activity. the activity is chosen from concrete experience. for example, doll figures representing the father, mother and child together with appropriate toy beds, a table and chairs, are presented to the child. these toys are presented one, two or three times at a time depending on the child's capacities. he is encouraged to relate the toys meaningfully; to place the father at the table, the baby in the bed, etc. this level of the activity reveals the child's capacities in inner language. some children require considerable training at this level because even their concrete daily experiences have not become logical. usually inner language is sufficiently developed so that he is ready to begin with receptive language when the parents bring him for training. in receptive language training the child is engaged in an activity as indicated above, and the therapist first gives him the symbol for objects he is using. this is referred to as the naming level; the child is given the names of the objects one at a time. for example, if he is engaged in putting the daddy figure in the bed, then the words daddy and bed would be given. timing is important. the symbol must be exactly appropriate to the act and the object at the time the child is performing it. after names of objects have been achieved, more complex language is introduced, such as "give the baby a ride" and "wash mary's hands". such directions should be given only when they can be done as part of the total play activity in which the child is engaged. during training on inner and receptive language, no demands are made to use expressive language (speech). after inner and receptive language have reached a level of practical usage, a third step — training in expressive language — is begun. children with basic problems of receptive aphasia, without a significant expressive involvement, begin using expressive language as soon as inner and receptive language have developed sufficiently. much emphasis on expressive language usually is unnecessary. thus training in expresive language is only for the expressive aphasics. the same principle as described above is used. the child is engaged in an activity and as he used a toy object, such as a car, the therapist asks, "what ir that?" this question should be a sincere request for information, not a demand. whatever utterance is given is accepted completely without correction. this naming of objects continued until some useful words or approximations have been achieved; the important factor is that the child's utterances become useful for communication rather than for perfection of articulation. the therapist then goes on to more complex language; as the child puts the toy dog in the car, she says, "where is the dog going?" the child can now reply by saying "bye-bye" or by using the phrases "go to the store," "for a ride", etc. the aphosie child's future parents and therapists frequently ask, "what shall wc expect from the aphasic child?" althoug specialized professional therapy for the aphasic child has only just begun, it is apparent that he has a good future. when parents learn more about his needs, when more therapists have been trained, when preschool and school age programmes have been developed, his future will be bright. such developments are forthcoming through the combined efforts of parents, teachers, psychologists, pediatricians, psychiatrists, neurologists, language pathologists, speech pathologists, audiologists and others. the aphasic child's problem is complex and difficult. this discussion has been an attempt to outline ways in which you can meet the challenge he presents. references: bender, l.: problems in conceptualization and communication in children with developmetal alexia, in psychopathology of communication, grune & stratton, new york, 1958. : psychopathology of children with organic brain disorders. c. c. thomas, springfield, 1956. eisenson, j . : examining for aphasia, psych. corp., new york, 1947. goldstein, k.: language and language disturbances. grune & stratton, new york 1948. gronnick, l.: aphasia, a guide in retraining. grune & stratton, new york, 1947. guttman, e.: aphasia in children. brain, 65, 205, 1942. head, h.: aphasia and kindred disturbances of speech cambridge university press, cambridge, 1926. myklebust, h. r.: aphasia in children, diagnosis and traning. in handbook of speech pathology. appleton-century crofts, new york, 1957. : auditory disorders in children. grune & stratton, new york, 1954. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) : the psychology of deafness. grune & stratton, new york, 1960. neilsen, j . : agnosia, aphaxia, aphasia. paul h. hoeber, new york, 1946. orton, s. t.: reading, writing and speech problems in children. w . w . norton, new york, 1937. penfield, w . : a consideration of the neurophysiological mechanisms of speech and some educational consequences. proc. amer. academy arts and sciences, 82, 5, 1953. penfield, w . and roberts l.: speech and brain mechanisms. princeton university press, princeton, 1959. piaget, j . : language and thought of the child. harcourt, brace, new york, 1926. strauss, a . and lehtinen, l.: psychopathology and education of the brain-injured child. grune and stratton, new york, 1947. wepman, j . : recovery from aphasia. ronald press, new york, 1951. wiesenberg, t . and mcbride, k.: aphasia. commonwealth fund, new york, 1953. june, 19631 journal of the south african logopedic society .13 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some comments on dysphonia jorge perell6, m . d . barcelona many speech therapists lack confidence when presented with a dysphonic patient. the following represent the writer's personal thoughts on the problem of dysphonia. 1. it is stated that the often read title treatment of dysphonia is something of a misnomer for the speech therapist. the dysphonia per se is not treated, but rather the disease which causes it. since there are over a hundred different diseases which cause dysphonia, it follows that treatment is varied. 2. before undertaking treatment of a dysphonic patient, the speech therapist must understand the diagnosis of the patient — but at no time must she herself attempt to diagnose the condition. diagnosis must be made by a suitably qualified medical practitioner. 3. with regard to the professional speaker, it is thought that laryngologists are too eager to refer these patients to speech therapists when the usual treatment of silence, antibiotics or vitamins has failed. the writer mentions that there are many small deviations — laryngeal micro pathology which too often escape the eye of an inexperienced laryngologist e.g. (a) a small alteration of the free edge of the vocal fold. (b) alteration in the structure of the mucous membrane covering the vocal fold. (c) alteration of the mobility of the vocal fold. when should a speech therapist treat a dysphonic patient ' 1. dysphonias produced by abuse of the voice in singing, shouting, incorrect voice production. this may result in hyperkinetic laryngopathy, hypokinetic laryngopathy, ventricular band voice, vocal nodule, vocal polyp or contact ulcer i.e. phonoponosis. however, in the case of vocal polyp or vocal nodule it is essential that the aggravating conditions be eliminated by a competent laryngologist prior to the commencement of voice therapy. 2. dysphonias caused by paralysis of the recurrent nerve, laryngeal trauma or scarring stenosis. in the foregoing cases vocal rehabilitation appears to improve voice. 3. dysphonias produced by alteration in the voice as a result of puberty. in such cases results are generally obtained quickly. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.14, nr. 1: sept. 6 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 36 jorge perello when should a speech therapist not treat a dysphonic patient i. when dysphonias are the result of: (a) spastic dysphonia. (b) obsessive dysphonia. (c) neurasthenic dysphonia. in these cases the speech therapist can help only if she works in close co-operation with a psychiatrist. even so, these patients are difficult to. rehabilitate. 2. aphonias which are caused by an hysterical condition. the author makes the controversial point that the above mentioned patients should be diagnosed and treated by a phoniatrist and never by a speech therapist. 3. dysphonias which do not become worse after speaking or singing. these may be of a serious nature and must be handled by a laryngologist. 4. dysphonias which deteriorate with silence. 5. there has been a tendency to refer dysphonic patients who present no laryngeal pathology to speech therapists who, at times, have little success with these patients. the author makes the interesting point that the dysphonia may be caused by some other physical pathology. three such cases are mentioned: i. in a dutch singer the dysphonia was caused by arthrosis of the cervical vertebrae. ii. in a south african singer the dysphonia was caused by androgenic medical treatment. iii. in a businessman the dysphonia was caused by diaphragmatic hernia. when the afore-mentioned cases were given vocal thearpy, little or no success resulted. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some misconceptions about infantile autism ε . b . p e l t z , b . a . hons. (rand)* " i c a n ' t believe t h a t ! " said alice. " c a n ' t y o u ? " the queen said in a pitying tone. " t r y again: d r a w a long b r e a t h and shut y o u r eyes." alice laughed. " t h e r e ' s n o use trying," she said: " o n e can't believe impossible things." " i d a r e say you h a v e n ' t had m u c h practice" said t h e q u e e n . " w h e n i was y o u r age i always did it for half an h o u r a day. w h y sometimes i've believed as m a n y as six impossible things before breakfast." 3 for therapists to be as level-headed as alice, it might help to know which impossible beliefs about infantile autism have been dissipated recently. theories of autism. the first large follow-up study of autistic children put great emphasis on the prognostic importance of speech at five years of age (kanner and eisenberg1"). in this study involving thirty-one subjects it appeared that all but one who were not speaking at the age of five were found to be feeble-minded, and/or grossly disturbed at adolescence or as adults. somehow this statistic has remained with us, leading either to therapeutic pessimism and premature institutionalization or providing an excuse for halfhearted therapy. this follow-up also disclosed that psychotherapy and other psychiatric treatments had no effect. more recently a large follow-up study by rutter, greenfeld and lockyer30 found that of thirty-one, five-year-old non-speakers, seven learned to speak after the age of five years. in all cases these children were receiving good schooling or speech therapy. the use of operant conditioning with autistic children of five to thirteen years has shown even more optimistic results, e.g. lovaas1 8 taught speech to a nine-year-old girl, pamela, who had previously undergone four years of unsuccessful psychoanalysis. pamela even learned to amuse herself with spontaneous activity when alone, e.g. drawing (lovaas et a l 1 0 ) . / jenson and womack1 4 were using milieu therapy, before turning to operant conditioning to teach speech to a seven-year-old boy. hewett1 0 taught a previously intractable thirteen-year-old autistic boy to read, so that he could eventually communicate by writing. can we afford to reserve judgment on conditioning until follow-ups are reported? * n o w at t h e institute of psychiatry, l o n d o n . journal of the south african logopedi society, vol. 16, no. , dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some misconceptions about infantile autism 37 in his first description of the autistic syndrome, kanner1 5 suggested that the primary defect was a disturbance in affective contact. on the other hand, rutter"' felt that autism is an impairment of language function. recently, many writers have claimed that it is caused by a cognitive or perceptual, rather than an affective difficulty. evidence for this thesis will be put forward later. rutter et al.: i t showed that the intelligence quotient of the autistic child is just as stable as in any other child and is a remarkably good predictor of the child's later intellectual, social and behavioural adjustment. social withdrawal, however, was found to diminish gradually with natural development, while children with the best outcomes in adolescence showed little change in intelligence quotient. in this study there were several children who remained completely without speech and yet had lost all evidence of social withdrawal. there are therapists who claim that although they have not succeeded over many years in teaching the autistic child speech, nevertheless they have reduced his autism. this is a wishful delusion. to evaluate therapy it is essential to know which symptoms are likely to remit spontaneously. rutter2 9 reported that: . . . it is striking that social withdrawal more than most symptoms tends to lessen considerably as the autistic child grows older. (p. 10) a major misconception which has been attacked is the interpretation of the autistic child's attraction to spinning objects. the autistic child can spend hours watching objects spin or twirling himself. hand flapping, which may be a related symptom, can take many forms, e.g. wiggling of the fingers, flicking at surfaces or oscillating of the hand. ornitz and ritvo2 1 considered hand flapping in response to a spinning top as diagnostic of autism. however, many autistic children later learn to suppress these behaviours. ornitz and ritvo2 2 mentioned that the vestibular system of the child may be pathologically involved. careful and detailed studies of the behaviour of autistic children (hand flapping in particular) have been carried out. sorosky et al. 3 3 showed that the average amounts of autistic behaviours over prolonged periods remain consistent. environmental variables were shown to have little or no effect on these behaviours. ritvo et al.2 7 felt that such findings could best be explained as resulting from an ongoing neuropathological process within the central nervous system. they showed that: (a) there is a statistically significant consistency in the frequencies of these behaviours among autistic children and within the same child. (b) once initiated, these behaviours are sustained at the same frequency, unaffected by fatigue and presumed changes in the ideational or affectual state. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 ε. β. peltz these motility patterns appeared in autistic children with varying degrees of severity of the disorder. ritvo et al. 2 7 postulated that the transient hand flapping and posturing seen in the startle reaction of the normal infant is never inhibited in the autistic child. they argued that there is an imbalance between excitation and inhibition mechanisms in the child. (another rational hypothesis is that these mannerisms have a self-stimulatory function.) ritvo et a l 2 7 strongly disagreed with the idea that withdrawal into fantasy could cause these behaviours, arguing that: all children regularly defecate and urinate and have fantasies about these behaviours. however, no-one would argue for the assumption that such fantasies actually produce the excrements in question. (p. 346) in view of the accumulating evidence towards a neurological basis for this behaviour, it would seem absurd to interpret flapping as a defence mechanism. i have seen one unfortunate autistic boy whose attraction to spinning objects was interpreted as a desire for the breast and for a long time he was allowed to unravel film and tape recorder reels undisturbed. this did not seem to satiate his longing. similar interpretations suggesting acting out therapy are quite detrimental, if one considers the consistent reinforcement of antisocial behaviour which this entails. another feature of infantile autism which has been over-burdened with naive interpretations is the autistic child's tendency to pronomial reversal. bettelheim1 claimed that autistic children will never echo / although they will echo other personal pronouns. this he related to their lack of a self-concept. rutter2 ! ) reported a study to test this inference, which found that when the position of the pronoun in the sentence was controlled, and obviously this is essential, autistic children did echo i as often as they echoed any other pronoun. the fact that / is rarely echoed under normal circumstances is simply a function of the fact that / usually comes at the beginning of a sentence, the part that is not commonly echoed. added evidence against a psycho-analytic interpretation of pronomial reversal is pointed out by fay 6 , i.e. that autistic children also fail to edit the demonstrative pronouns this and these to that and those. fay's autistic subject showed many more echoisms and errors in response to questions in which the carrier phrase was show me the than questions in which the phrase was point to the the writer has confirmed this feature of the comprehension of the autistic child on another child. surely linguistic or conceptual explanations are far more parsimonious than those in terms of aggression and rejection towards the interlocutor, j bettelheim1 has also claimed that the autistic child's lack of high level performance is the result of disinclination, which masks an journal of the south african logopedie society, vol, 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some misconceptions about infantile autism 39 average or above-average level of intelligence. hingtgen and churchill11 showed that when motivational levels were elevated and when previously learned behaviours were being emitted consistently, autistic children still experienced extreme difficulty in learning low-level tasks. undoubtedly it is true that most autistic children are retarded in language development and that many never overcome these conceptual difficulties thus remaining without abstract language (rutter29). in view of these facts it is astonishing to note the tendency to interpret the autistic child's action to him during therapy. brawley et al.2 have shown that remarks like you don't have to hit yourself, steve. do you feel angry with yourself? was that (task) too hard?, actually increase the frequency of the undesirable behaviour. remarks that i have heard, e.g. are you feeling a little mixed-up today?, are probably even more unintelligible to the child. the parents of the autistic child are reputedly detached, refrigerated and obsessive, but some writers have not found this a consistent trait (rutter20). even if this is granted for the moment certain difficulties arise: (a) mothers may become detached and ambivalent in response to their disturbed child. (b) autistic children rarely have a history of the deprived circumstances of the institutionalised child. (c) it would have to be a very severe parental abnormality indeed to cause so gross a disorder as infantile autism in so short a time. it is always dangerous to argue from correlation to cause, and until some factual surveys appear, we should avoid the all too frequent judgments of the autistic child's parents. the early descriptions of autistic children also found that their parents were from the academic and professional communities. however, the broader clinical experience of the next two decades has shown that these children come from every socio-economic class. ornitz and ritvo2 1 reported that: while some of the parents are reported to be cold, isolated or refrigerated individuals, others have proven to be warm, loving and quite capable of raising normally affectionate siblings of their autistic child. a condition of family disruption and emotional turmoil may surround the infancy or childhood of autistic children or the disease process may develop in a normal emotional climate. (p. 78) autism as a cognitive dysfunction. rimland2 3 claimed that the autistic child is grossly impaired in a function basic to all cognition; the ability to relate new stimuli to remembered experience. new sensation can only be related to sharply limited fragments of memory, and the child therefore cannot derive meaning from his tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 40 ε. β. peltz expsrience. consequently he cannot understand relationships, nor think in terms of concepts, symbols, analogies or abstractions. he cannot integrate his sensations into a comprehensible whole; his perception of the world is vague and obscure. however, on tasks in which the input does not have to be re-organised, the autistic child can show high-level performance, e.g. form boards. strong evidence for such a hypothesis is provided by the autistic child's pattern of cognitive abilities (rutter28). characteristically the child's immediate memory is good, but on tests of verbal concepts, abstraction or symbolization he is poor. this also applies to tests which do not require the child to speak. there is no evidence that the mutism is in any way elective. rutter 2 8 noticed in the maudsley series that the peaks of intelligence of the autistic child were found almost exclusively on the block design and object assembly tests of the wechsler intelligence scale for children, probably because these tests require the least in the way of comprehension of instructions. we have already discussed the facts from follow-up studies which support a language dysfunction hypothesis, viz. that the language difficulties must be considered as a primary phenomenon, not as a feature secondary to social withdrawal. rather, it is the comprehension defects which might lead to withdrawal. the typical lack of response to sounds, the frequency with which the autistic child is considered to be deaf and the difficulties with spoken instruction suggest a relationship between autism and receptive aphasia. rutter 2 9 mentioned that the audiometric responses of autistic children and of children with receptive aphasia are the same. possibly the autistic child's problems are not restricted to the auditory modality. while the aphasic's non-verbal responses are superior to those which require verbal input or output, the autistic child shows less scatter. davis4 felt that the language deficits in autistic children were directly related to the pervasive concreteness of all their behaviour. in a series of experiments, o'connor and hermelin have been unravelling the nature of the conceptual dysfunction. they have repeatedly highlighted the difficulty autistic children have in responding to structure in their environments. / these dysfunctions, as shown in recall tests, included: / (a) syntactic structure: hermelin3 demonstrated that autistic children recalled sentences and random arrangements of words equally well, while normal and sub-normal controls could only match their recall of sentences. the autistic children tended to remember the last part of any message whether sense or nonsense, while the controls regrouped the words, recalling the most meaningful parts. this throws light on a feature of autism, viz. echolalia. ι journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some misconceptions about infantile autism 41 (b) semantic structure: when a series of words from two conceptual categories were presented (e.g. blue, horse, pig, red, dog, yellow), the normals and subnormals showed significantly more reclustering into concepts and thus higher recall than the autistics. (c) phonetic stress: under one condition sentences were presented with stress on the key words, under the second condition stress was on connecting words. it was found that autistics recalled the stressed words under both conditions, while normals recalled the key words. further experiments, with binary sequences, showed that autistic children have difficulty in detecting structure in any modality. when errors were analysed, the normals were found to have detected the dominant features of the sequences, while the errors of the autistics were random. this confirmed earlier work by o'connor and hermelin20 which showed that in learning tasks, autistic children, particularly those without speech, had difficulty in processing information given to them, while subnormal controls did not exhibit these difficulties. hermelin7 emphasised the autistic child's relative inability to vary his response according to the stimulus. reinforcement therapy. traditional psychodynamic methods of therapy, in all their diversity, have failed to demonstrate any reasonable degree of efficacy with psychotic children . . . . on the other hand the rapidly growing body of literature reporting results of behaviour modification methods attests to the potential power of this 'new' therapy. (leff,11 p. 397). together with this new approach has come a realization of the importance of speech therapy in the remedial programme for the autistic child. savage31 stated that intensification of normal speech stimulation methods produces poor results and operant conditioning is favoured by her as the most satisfactory behavioural approach. often, papers from practitioners of psychodynamic therapies explicitly incorporate basic tenets of reinforcement theory in their therapies. weiland and rudnick3 s made an eight-year-old autistic boy's receipt of his favourite toy contingent upon his verbalization of the word ball, widening the child's vocabulary until he was eventually able to sing songs. dubnoff5 successfully treated an autistic boy and reported: h e is expected to verbalize his demands before they are gratified. the demands and expectations are commensurate with what a child is able to produce at a given time and only appropriate behaviour is gratified. (p. 386) such therapy is a long way from an all-accepting permissive and indiscriminately rewarding psycho-analytic approach, and probably tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 ε. β. peltz the best therapy will evolve from collaboration. nevertheless leff17 cautioned that: it is in practiccs such as these that many therapists, either wittingly or unwittingly often make effective use of the basic principles of operant learning. the evidence suggests that therapeutic potency is a direct function of the degree to which these principles are systematically applied. (p. 403) perhaps the simplest hypothetical explanation for the unique success of behaviour modification techniques is that the autistic child is unable to learn under ordinary circumstances, (probably the result of some form of cognitive dysfunction). unfortunately, however, even more misconceptions than those attached to autism have shrouded the potential of reinforcement therapy. bettelheim's1 picture of an adult forcing responses from a robotlike child during conditioning is shared by laymen and dogmatic therapists alike. rimland24 confesses that when writing his book he was similarly prejudiced. however, he has since become a champion of operant conditioning and the lovaas school. hingtgen and churchill11 mentioned that sessions were characterized by a happy smiling child performing consistently. while training a thirteen-year-old autistic boy to read, hewett1 0 noted that his subject's interest in his education steadily grew despite gradual decreases in reinforcement. the child showed enjoyment of his work by laughing and vocalizing. he also frequently initiated new learning tasks by bringing the teacher pictures whose symbolic designation he was eager to master. the consistency and clarity of the operant situation allows even the autistic child an experience of mastery. the therapist who is thoroughly conversant with the reinforcement technique can broaden the operant approach greatly, e.g. schell et al.3 2 encouraged a game in which kipper, their autistic subject, returned a non-preferred cereal reinforcer while the therapist said no, no. then the therapist said candy, yes while giving the preferred candy. after twelve trials, kipper was saying no, no when given the cereal and a few times said kiki when the clinician asked what he wanted. other play activities included tickling and cuddling games· which caused psals of laughter. kipper was encouraged to say up' or down for being lifted into the air or slid down the therapist's knees. these verbal responses were shown to be dependent on their rewarding consequences. (more formal training! was also used.) a criticism of conditioning is that, it is situation-specific, i.e. it does not generalize. hingtgen and trost' 3 found that after training autistic children to initiate contact and vocal behaviour with each other in an experimental situation, these new behaviours were observed to journal of the south african logopedi society, vol 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some misconceptions about infantile autism 43 increase in the ward setting as well as in the home. hewett1 0 and jenson and womack1 1 reported spontaneous speech developing once transfer out of the experimental situation was effected. an essential feature of reinforcement therapy is the training of the parents in techniques of conditioning. many parents have thus been enabled to take over the training of their autistic child — usually with great success, (e.g. wolf et al.,3" risley and wolf25). this allows the autistic child to be reintegrated into the family and to enter into natural communities of reinforcement. many atavistic behaviours have proved amenable to negative reinforcement (most often time out from positive reinforcement). tantrums have repeatedly been found to be maintained by adult attentiveness to them. often it is only when these behaviours are eliminated that teaching programs can begin. in this area we also find many therapists indulging a lay protest reaction against punishment techniques, but remember the far greater pain that we subject a child to without hesitation, when it is necessary to remove his tonsils or appendix. helen keller was lucky her teacher did not insist on using only hugs and kisses. if she had, helen keller would have ended up as a living vegetable. in practice, however, the reinforcement programme is heavily loaded with smiles, cuddles and sweets. punishment (except time out from positive reinforcement) is avoided as it involves many uncontrolled side effects. a great advantage of operant conditioning is the speed with which large improvements can be obtained. hingtgen et a l 1 2 taught two mute autistic children, over five years of age, who had not responded to all other therapy, to do the following, within three weeks: (a) use of the body — including imitation of blowing, crossing feet, chewing, etc. (b) use of objects — including imitated line drawing, folding paper, cutting with scissors, brushing teeth, etc. (c) vocal responses — seventeen sounds and eleven words were learned by the five-and-a-half year old girl, eighteen sounds and eighteen words by the six-and-a-half year old boy. this efficiency has never been approached by other therapies. in order to evaluate experiments of operant conditioning, a special research design is used in which successive measures of the same subject's behaviour are taken. the subject is his own control, in that his base-rate behaviour, recorded prior to therapy, is compared with his behaviour after the experimental manipulation. temporary discontinuance of the modification procedure (usually the reinforcement) tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 ε. β. peltz is used typically to verify the effects of this procedure. such experimental designs can provide immediate and conclusive demonstrations of efficacy and are far superior to the anecdotal reports from other schools. to aid the autistic child, the reinforcement therapist breaks the tasks into infinitesimally small steps, each of which the child can master. however, it is essential to remember that these ingenious programmes are closely related to the reinforcement technique. 1 have seen a therapist, using the method of an operant experimenter, without using reinforcement. such practices are rather dubious considering that almost every article cn operant conditioning is at pains to demonstrate that the reinforcement is the only operative variable. in the field of infantile autism, the mutual interests of the speech therapist and the psychologist can best be served by close co-operation. the structuring techniques derived from speech therapy with the aphasic child can be applied fruitfully within the conditioning situation. davis4 suggests that one should control the stimulus modality by blocking others that might interfere, e.g. by covering the child's eyes and talking directly into his ear. further, the complexity of the task should be controlled by regulating the number of objects present, e.g. saying one word which the subject must associate with the only object in a clear visual field or a clearly defined movement. operant training derives its success from the fact that not only are specific behaviours taught, but the child learns how to focus and direct his attention. a promising development is the recent tendency of behaviour therapists to teach learning sets or strategies to autistic children. risley and wolf2li expanded their autistic subjects' vocabulary to phrases and sentences. they were then taught to generalize the grammatical form with appropriate substitutions, which could be called generative speech. there is little limit to the breadth of activities which can be taught through reinforcement. savage31 wrote: many people disagree with operant conditioning on the rather nebulous grounds of 'inhumanity'. h o w much less humane is it to deny a child an opportunity to acquire the tools of communication when the alternative is probably long-term care in a psychiatric hospital? people have objected to the method, suggesting that it does not teach language, but produces robot-like speech. in a m u t e autistic child, words acquired through operant conditioning form the basis of a language pattern. without this basis, a child will never learn language skills as a means of communication. with it. there is a chance that h e might. (p. 87) note: detailed descriptions of speech therapy using operant conditioning can be found in lovaas1", hewett1 1, risley and wolf2,;; stark et al.3 4 and brawley et al.2. journal of the south african logopedi society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some misconceptions about infantile autism 45 summary recent studies throw light on certain widespread misconceptions about infantile autism and offer rational alteratives: (a) autism should not be considered a disturbance in affective contact but a cognitive and perceptual difficulty. this difficulty, rather than social withdrawal, is the central feature of the disorder. linguistic and problem solving studies tend to confirm this probability. (b) pre-occupation with hand flapping and spinning objects in autistic children does not require interpretation, as recent neurological studies indicate the strong probability of a neurological basis for this tendency. (c) a highly permissive approach is not likely to help the autistic child and may intensify atavistic behaviours. to hold parents of autistic children culpable is unjustified. progress in therapy and in the home is probably related to a systematic use of reinforcement. (d) the absence of speech by the age of five years no longer means a hopeless prognosis, as operant conditioning allows speech to be learned after this age. dealing with autism as a cognitive dysfunction, rimland-3 emphasises the child's inability to comprehend his environment and to integrate his sensations, while rutter-'j shows the similarity between autism and receptive aphasia. he claims that social withdrawal is secondary to language difficulties in autism. for this reason and because the autistic child seems unable to learn under ordinary circumstances, it seems that reinforcement therapy practised within the framework of speech therapy would show the best results. operant conditioning has been unjustly attacked on many accounts, but the hopeful prognosis and breadth of applicability it offers should be a spur towards dissipation of these misconceptions. opsomming onlangse studies werp lig op sekere algemene wanopvattings oor infantiele outisme en bied rasionele alternatiewe: (a) outisme moet nie beskou word as 'n versteuring in affektiewe kontak nie, maar wel as 'n kognitiewe en perseptuele afwyking. laasgenoemde is dan die sentrale kenmerk van die versteuring en nie soseer sosiale onttrekking nie. linguistieseen probleemoplossingstudies ondersteun hierdie moontlikheid. (b) die beheptheid van die outistiese kind met handgeswaai en draaiende voorwerpe benodig nie interpretasie nie. onlangse neurotydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 46 ε. β. peltz logiese studies toon 'η sterk moontlikheid van 'n neurologiese basis vir hierdie neiging. (c) 'n baie toelaatbare benadering word nie aanbeveel nie aangesien dit eerder atavistiese gedrag versterk. om ouers aanspreeklik te hou vir die kind se probleem is onregverdig en ongegrond. vooruitgang in terapie en ook tuis is blykbaar verwant aan die sistematiese gebruik van versterking. (d) die afwesigheid van spraak by 'n ouderdom van vyf jaar beteken nie rneer 'n hopelose prognose nie — operante kondisicnering maak voorsiening vir spraakaanleer selfs na hierdie ouderdom. by 'n beskouing van outisme as 'n kognitiewe disfunksie beklemtoon rimland2 1 die kind se onvermoe om sy omgewing te begryp en om sy sensasies te integreer. rutter™ wys op die ooreenkoms tussen outisme en reseptiewe afasie en beweer dat die sosiale onttrekkmg sekonder is tot die taalprobleme. hierdie is een rede waarom kondisioneringsterapie, beoefen binne die raamwerk van spraakterapie, die beste resultate toon, 'n verdere rede is dat die outistiese kind blykbaar nie die vermoe besit om onder gewone omstandighede te leer nie. operante kondisionering is al dikwels onbillik aangeval, maar die hoopvolle prognose en die toepassingsgeleenthede wat dit bied, behoort as aansporing te dien om hierdie opvattings uit die weg te ruim. references l bettelheim, b. (1967): the empty fortress. collier-macmillan, london. 2. brawley, e. r., harris. f . r.. allen, η. κ. e., fleming, r. s. and peterson, r. f. (1969): behaviour modification of an autistic child. behavioural science. 14. 87-97. 3. carrol, lewis: through the looking glass (1950): max parrish and co., london. , , 4. davis. b. j. (1968): a clinical method of appraisal of the language and learning behaviour of young autistic children. journal of communication disorders. 1, 277-296. . . 5. dubnoff. b. (1965): the habituation and education of the autistic child in a therapeutic day school. american journal of orthopsychiatry, 35, 385-386. , , ^ 6. fay, w. h. (1969): on the basis of autistic echolalia. journal of communication disorders, 2, 38-47. . . . 7. hermelin, b. (1966): psychological research. early childhood autism, (ed.) j. k. wing. pergammon press, oxford. 8. hermelin, b. (1967): coding and immediate recall in autistic children. proceedings of the royal society of medicine, 60, 563-564. / 9. hermelin. b. and o'connor, n. (1964): visual imperception.an psychotic children. british journal of psychology, 56, 455-460. 7 10 hewett. f. m. (1965): teaching speech to an autistic child through operant conditioning. american journal of orthopsychiatry, 35, 927-936. 11. hingtgen. j. n. and churchill, d. w. (in press): differentia! effects of behaviour modification in four mute autistic boys. 12. hingtgen. j. n., coulter, s. k. and churchill, d. w. (1967): intensive reinforcement of imitative behaviour in mute autistic children. archives of general psychiatry, 17, 36-43. , 13. hingtgen, j. n . and trost, f . c. (196(4): shaping co-operative responses in early childhood schizophrenics: ii reinforcement of mutual physical contact and vocal responses. presented at1 american psychological association. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 16, no. , dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some misconceptions about infantile autism 47 14. jensen, g. d. and womack, m. g. (1967): operant conditioning techniques applied in the treatment of an autistic child. american journal of orthopsychiatry, 37, 30-34. 15. kanner. l. (1943): autistic disturbances of affective contact. nervous child, 2, 217-250. 16. kanner, l. and eisenberg, l. (1955): notes on the follow-up studies of autistic children. psychopathology of childhood: (eds.) p. h. hoch & j. zubin. grune and stratton, new york. 17. left, r. (1968): behaviour modification and the psychoses of childhood: a review. psychological bulletin, 69, 396-409. 18. lovaas, i. (1966): a program for the establishment of speech in psychotic children. early childhood autism. (ed.) j. k. wing, pergammon press, oxford. 19. lovaas, l, freitas, l., nelson, k. & whalen, c. (1967): the establishment of imitation and its use for the development of complex behaviour in schizophrenic children. behaviour research and therapy, 5, 171-181. 20. o'connor, n. and hermelin, b. (1965): sensory dominance in autistic, children and controls. archives of general psychiatry, 12, 99-103. 21. ornitz, ε. m. and ritvo, e. r. (1968a): perceptual inconstancy in early infantile autism. archives of general psychiatry, 18, 76-98. 22. ornitz, ε. m. and ritvo, e. r. (1968b): neurophysiologic mechanisms underlying perceptual inconstancy in autistic and schizophrenic children. archives of general psychiatry. 19, 22-21. 23. rimland, b. (1964): infantile autism. methuen, london. 24. rimland, b. (1965): breakthrough in the treatment of mentally ill children. reprint of a lecture. 25. risley, t. r. and wolf, μ. m. (1964): experimental manipulation of autistic behaviours and generalisation into the home. child development: readings in experimental analysis, (eds. s. w. bijou and d. m. baer). appleton-century-crofts, new york. 26. risley, j. and wolf m. (1967): establishing functional speech in echolalic children. behaviour research and therapy. 5, 73-88. 27. ritvo, e. r., ornitz, ε. m. and franchi, s. (1968): frequency of repetitive behaviours in early infantile autism and its variants. archives of general psychiatry, 19, 341-347. 28. rutter, m. (1966: behavioural and congnitive characteristics. early childhood autism (ed.) j. k. wing. pergammon press, oxford. 29. rutter, m. (1968): concepts of autism: a review of research. journal of child psychology and psychiatry, 9, 1-25. 30. rutter, m., greenfeld, d. and lockyer, l. (1967): a five-year to fifteen year follow-up study of infantile psychosis. 11 social and behavioural outcome. the british j o u r n a l of psychiatry, 113, 1183-1199. 31. savage, v. a. (1968): childhood autism: a review of the literature with particular reference to the speech and language structure of the autistic child. british journal of disorders of communication, 3, 75-87. 32. schell, r. e., stark, j. and giddon, j. j. (1967): development of language behaviour in an autistic child. journal of speech and hearing disorders 32, 51-64. 33. sorosky, a. d., ornitz, ε. m., brown, μ. b. and ritvo, e. r. (1968): systematic observations of autistic behaviour. archives of general psychiatry, 18, 439-449. 34. stark, j., giddan, j. j. and meisel, j. (1968): increasing verbal behaviour in an autistic child. journal of speech and jiearing disorders, 33, 42-47. 35. weiland, i. h. and rudnik, r. (1961): considerations of the development and treatment of autistic childhood psychosis. the psychoanalytic studv of the child, 16, 549-563. 36. wolf, m.. risley, t. r. and mees, h. (1964): application of operant conditioning procedures to the behaviour problems of an autistic child. behaviour research and therapy, 1, 305-312. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) june journal of the south african logopedic society tests designed to discover potential reading difficulties at the six-year old level katrina de hirsch, l.c.s.t. director, pediatric language disorder clinic, pediatric section, vunderbill clinic, columbia presbyterian medical centre, new york city. it is a well-known fact that our schools carry a fairly large percentage of educational and emotional casualties, bright: children whose life at school is a burden because they suffer from a more or less severe reading disability. we have only to look at the intake of a pediatric-psychiatric " ' dinic or at a sample of the youngsters referred to our child guidance centers to find a sizable number of intelligent children whose somatic complaints or vbehavioral disturbances developed only after they, had been exposed to the experience of continued failure at school. since remedial facilities in our private and public; educational institutions are few and far between, we shall have to find ways to prevent the occurrence of reading failure. in order to do this we need tools which enable us to predict with reasonable certainty which youngsters are liable to run into trouble in the first and second grades. once we select these children we may find that some of them simply need more time in which to mature, but that others might do very well, if given specific techniques, right from the start. careful selection and c o n s i s t e n t planning on our part might easily prevent a great deal of heartache and frustration later on. how then can we find out at the end of the kindergarten year, in the five-to-six-year-old group — and i stress this time since it is crucial as far as certain maturational processes are concerned — which children are liable to find the going rough? the remarks offered here are of an entirely tentative nature. at the' pediatric language disorder clinic, columbia-presbyterian medical center, we believe that we have evolved some procedures designed to predict future reading performance and we think that in a fair percentage of oases our prediction has been correct. we have not as yet done a statistical evaluation, but having experimented along certain lines, we are now trying to find out where such experimentation will lead us. we use the well-known intelligence tests as an over-all measurement of the child's basic intellectual endowment. among them the bellevuewechsler scale for children seems to be the most satisfactory. however, in a large percentage of cases these tests do not predict future success or failure in reading, s p e l l i n g , and writing. the better-known reading readiness tests, which we also use, do not seem to us to cover all the facets of behavior which we think are significant. the metropolitan readiness test, is probably the; best since it stresses comprehension not only of single words, but also of more complex verbal units. it does not, however, test ability to use verbal material and it fails to evaluate a variety of aspects which enter into reading performance. we agree with gillingham's observation that children with a mental· age under 6£ are not ready for the printed word (5). reading readiness is a function of development. we look on development, emotional and neurophysiological, as a progressive increase in complexity of behavioral patterns. studies on normal development show that psychological functioning and cerebral organization reveal a steady increase in differentiation and integration through adolescence. as the child grows older he has to cope with increasingly more differentiated and highly integrated organizations. among these more complex skills is the ability to use verbal tools. at the age of six children are supposed to have mastered oral symbols. we expect them to have organized an enormous number of'arbitrary phonetic signs into the pattern of language, a formidable achievement which by no means all children have accomplished at this age, as evidenced by the numerous youngsters who still show infantile speech patterns. once a child reaches first grade he is expected to cope with a secondary symbolic system, with visual signs which have to be correlated with meaning. in order to read a little word like "hat," a sequence of letters seen, a sequence in space has to be translated into a sequence of sounds heard, a sequence in time. we fed that without a measure of maturation — perceptual, motor, • the author wants to express her gratitude to dr. william s. i-angford for his constant and unfailing help and guidance. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society june conceptual and behavioral maturation — the child will be unable to cope with this task. the youngster whose neurophysiological organization still is primitive, the one whose language equipment is inferior, is the one who will probably ruu into trouble in the first , and second grades. it is this often very intelligent child whom we have to tingle out before he is exposed to reading failure children between 5$ and usua'ly make dramatic strides in over-all maturation, strides so dramatic, in fact, tlio· one occasionally feels one can literally see them blossoming forth. the best time to test them, therefore, is at the end of their year in kindergarten. since maturation and development involve the whole child, we observe the youngster's total behavioi in order to determine reading readiness. we usually direct our attention first to those who have trouble with integration of coordination. movement, like perception, requires patterning. a certain level of motor skills is not only essential for learning to write and print, but it is also indicative of the child's over-all maturity. the youngsters i refer to do not usually show the severe deviations in large muscular control which we find in cerebral palsied children, but they sometimes have trouble throwing a ball, riding a bicycle, skipping rope. we ask our children to throw darts, to walk on their heels, to hop on one foot. we do occasionally find some who not only have difficulty with the execution of these movements, but who also fail, as in ideomotor apraxia, to get the idea of the act itself. their over-all motility is often like that of younger children, and it is of interest that bender and yarnell (2) have commented on motility disturbances in children suffering from various forms of language disabilities. some have not attained the neurological maturation which enables them to execute movements of specific muscle groups. they retain some of the characteristics of the global, total motor response which is typical for the very young child. for instance, they turn the whole head when asked to flex the tongue. lags in integration and patterning of finer muscular control and a degree of dyspraxia, as described by orton (12), are observed relatively frequently, though occasionally this dyspraxia is confined to graphic activities. oseretsky's tests (4) provide an extensive survey of muscular skills and have been standardized in terms of normal development. ttiey are thus useful in determining in which areas a child's performance lags. unfortunately, these tests are difficult to administer and for practical purposes we have to rely on our own observations. we give our children identical tasks to perform in order to get a basis for comparison. we watch for jerkiness and arrhythmicity in the smaller muscles of the hand and tongue. the way in which a child handles construction toys will not only reveal his manual dexterity and the fluidity of his movements, but it also provides opportunities for observing many other facets of his behaviour: his span of attention, his frustration threshold, his curiosity and his zest. research of the last ten years — especially werner's work (17) — has revealed the close relationship between perceptual and motor functioning. basic to reading, of course, is the child's ability to cope with perceptual organization. the very young child normally has difficulty in breaking up the totality of a pattern; his perceptual organization is somewhat diffuse; single parts are poorly differentiated. however, the differentiation of small details and the understanding of the essential relationships between the parts and the whole are a sine qua non in reading. in the metropolitan readiness test we find a few items which require the child to discriminate between small visual details. in fact, much of the reading readiness work done in kindergarten and in the earlier part of the first grade is dedicated to the training of such discrimination, and any of the books used in this readiness work can be used for testing. the bender gestalt test (1), designed for evaluation of visuomotor functioning, is one of the most important in our battery. visually perceived configurations are offered to children with the request that they be copied. obviously the infant does not experience perception as the adult does. but the child who is expected to read and write must have visuomotor experiences similar to those of the adult. bender says the evolution of visuomotor gestalten is a maturational, not an educative process. it is true that the average 6-year-old does not usually copy all the figures correctly. developmentally there is a progression in the performance of the copied patterns from a controlled scribble at age 3 to all figures clearly perceived and reproduced at age 11. however, as silver (13) points out, it is of interest that many of our highly intelligent dyslexic children are unable to cope with this task even at 12 or 13. they are unable to grasp or retain visual patterns made up of discrete elements. in our children we not only observe difficulty with the handling of the pencil and trouble with manual control, but we also see immature forms (loops, perhaps, instead of dots) which are characteristic for the ages of 4 and 5. we note verticalization of horizontally oriented figures. we find, in other words, inability to correctly perceive and reproduce given configurar ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) june journal of the south african logopedic society tions — functions required in the reading process. many of our youngsters have great difficulty spacing these figures on paper. the ability to cope with spatial relationships is of primary importance since in reading and writing the child has to deal with a pattern laid out in space. a developmental lag in this area will thus show up in the youngster's reading performance. notions of space originally derive from the child's consciousness of his own body. we use the goodenough draw-a-man test (8) not as an intelligence measurement, nor as a way to evaluate the child's image of self in the emotional sense, though both are of interest. we use it primarily as a relatively reliable indioator of the child's body image, a concept which refers to his awareness of parts of his own body and their relationship to each other. this image is dosely related to spatial concepts and is often strikingly immature and primitive in the type of child we are discussing here. awareness of left and right, of course, is a significant aspect of the child's notion of space. since the ability to cope with the specific directional discipline of left to right progression is required for reading in our culture, this aspect deserves further discussion. as described earlier, we carefully watch the child while we test his finer muscular coordination. since most of the activities in which he is engaged are untrained and are not influenced by early conditioning, they are useful in determining the degree to which a youngster has established a functional superiority of one hand over the other. failure to establish such superiority may be related to familial factors; it may also indicate physiological immaturity and thus tend to show up in reading. we take note of early attempts to switch handedness as well as of family history with regard to laterality. in testing we evaluate strength, precision and speed. we carefully note eye dominance since crossed laterality may adversely influence reading performance. in the small child, awareness of concepts of right and left progresses slowly. the average child of six can demonstrate right and left on his own body — usually with the help of gestural (motor) responses — but not on anybody else's. that is why head's finger-to-eye test (9) (originally devised for brain-injured individuals and first used with children by simon (14) ), which calls for mirror imitation of the examiner's movements, had to be discarded in this form. it seemed to us that the test requires a level of abstraction which the six-yearold has not yet attained. ttie v. tk e c^omplimenti of ittidual disfruxitws (pty.) ltd. specialist suppliers of physiotherapy and rehabilitation equipment now at "cape york" telephone 252 jeppe street 23-8106 johannesburg p.o. box 3378 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 journal of the south african logopedic society junechild of six* however, should be able to imitate one's movements when sitting alongside one and watching one's gestures in a mirror. we have observed that a good many children who fail in this respect later develop reading difficulties. the horst's tests (14), which we include in our battery, are useful because they give us a clue as to whether or not the youngster is able to discriminate between identical shapes when they are presented in correct and in reversed form. spatial and directional concepts are not the only ones which are pertinent in reading. language, spoken or printed, is laid out in a time-space pattern. thus we have to investigate not only spatial but also temporal organization. it is of interest that a number of workers, like stambak (15) and mottier (11), have consistently found rhythmic difficulties in children suffering from reading disabilities, especially in those whose oral language is already somewhat insecure. we ask our youngsters to imitate tapped-out patterns of v a r y i n g difficulty and have observed that a goodly percentage of them fail in the repetition of even short and simple sequences. rhythm is a configuration in time, and fundamentally our children have trouble with all types of configurations. we further require that our children repeat a series of rionsense syllables," and find in the large majority of cases that they have strikingly short auditory memory spans. the correlation of this feature with language disability seems especially high. the child of six should be able to repeat at least four or five syllables, but most of our youngsters manage just three. there is another area which has so far received little attention and which is closely related to perceptual organization. we know that brain injury, and in fact any lowering of integrative efficiency, brings about an impairment in figurebackground relationships. weaknesses in figureground relationships have riot been systematically explored in children with severe reading disabilities. however, the indications are that these youngsters, though to a lesser degree than do brain-injured ones, have difficulties in this area. in order to cope with spoken or printed language the child must be able to pick out the figure from the background. for one to interpret a sentence heard (the spoken configuration), the message (like the tapped-out pattern) must stand out clearly. for one to decipher a printed sentence, the configuration must be well defined and sharply delineated against the page. i have seen innumerable youngsters look at printed material as if it represented a meaningless design. only if the figure does stand out_ will the sentence or the phrase have structure, or, in other words, meaning. some of our children have trouble separating figure from background. they do not discover, for instance, the significant design in one of the puzzles when asked to find the lion in the jungle. if one gives them raised geometric figures to reproduce graphically from touch, one often finds that they are drawn to the background, to the roughness of the cloth, for instance. the marble board test, which was originally designed to evaluate figure background relationships in brain-injured children, is difficult to administer. at the clinic we therefore use an adaptation of the figure background cards presented in strauss and lehtinen's book on brain-injured child (16). it has been established by goldstein's work (6) that a certain measure of abstract functioning is a requisite of language performance. the question then arises, what does abstract functioning mean at this early age? we know that the formation of abstract relationships is a developmental process which starts with perceptual and configurational relationships and develops in the direction of conceptual classification. this applies to both nonverbal and verbal behaviour. the small child who tries to use the toy toilet have you read these important books? westerman & ward: practical phonetics for students of south african languages. van riper: speech correction, principles & methods. anderson: improving the child's speech. mccullogh: speech improvement work and practice book university bookshop central news agency ltd. phineas court 34 bertha street braamfontein. phone 44-7523: 44-5185 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) june journal of the south african logopedic society for his own use while playing with it has not vet understood that a toy only represents the real thing. this experience a nonverbal one, comes later it is a tremendous step forward when the child first pretends to be a 4iurse or a pilot. some children are relat i v e l y concrete at the age of six. practically all brain-injured children are. but we find the group which has difficulties with various aspects of language function to be similarly concrete — if to a lesser degree — and this concreteness is by no means restricted to the verbal area. in order to test abstract behaviour on a nonverbal level, we give our youngsters block designs to copy, and watch whether they are able to analyze wholes into parts and as the next step bo synthesize these parts into wholes. guided by the goldstein-scherer tests (7), we give them a variety of objects to sort out, and observe whether they are able to isolate eating utensils, "things to eat with," so as to find out whether they have some form of categorical behaviour, at least on a perceptual level. the testing discussed up to this point has been confined to non-verbal tasks; we go from these to verbal ones. the ability to handle verbal tools is basic to reading. in order to cope with visual symbols the child must have mastered auditory ones, oral language. careful testing in this area often reveals significant gaps which are frequently overlooked. authors like orton (12), mccarthy (10), and borell-maisonny (3) have long stressed the close relationships between reading efficiency and oral language skills. first of all we have to make sure that the youngster fully understands spoken materia. units like "in front of," "inside," "beneath, which are fairly abstract concepts, are by no means always as securely established as we are • inclined to think. we place the child in front ol the dollhouse and suggest he put the baby next to the bathtub and so on. carrying out complex directions is not always easy for our children; many are unable to interpret a somewhat involved story; as a matter of fact, a few are not ready to listen at! all. some do not catch on when presented with an absurdity couched in verbal terms, although they are easily able to see the point if the absurd is presented in pictorial form. comprehension of language is one thing; use of language, another. we carefully check on artidilatory patterns! we note length of units and listen for difficulties in word finding. there are children who have a relatively good use of the idiom on an auditory perceptual basis; they sound as though they have a large vocabulary but some of them fail when asked to give a word on being presented with a picture. we are interested in vocal patterns — an unusual degree of monotony may reftect a difficulty with structuralization, perhaps a figurebackground problem. the child's ability to form correct grammatical constructions is of importance. grammar is an expression of structure, and the child who leaves out small connectives in the sentence may have difficulties with the temporal, spatial and causal relationships expressed by these words. we further want to know whether the youngster is able to tell a simple story and to bring out its salient features. some children's organization of verbal material is so poor that they never get their point across! they get so involved in the intricacies of the "three little pigs" that they ramble on indefinitely. in the beginning the child's spoken language is on a very concrete level. a three-year-old who says "brush" does not refer to the category "brush," to the object whose essential qualities are unchanging from situation to ^ situation. "brush" to him might mean one time, "brush my hair"; another time, "the brush is on die table." he does not use the word in a categorical sense. in testing abstract functioning on a verbal level we look for the youngster's ability to classify and categorize. we ask him to name all diningroom furniture. if he includes the wallpaper or the silver dishes on the sideboard he shows thereby that he has not yet understood the category "furniture." werner (18) cites the example of the boy who includes bread and a pipe with the bench, the saw and the hammer when asked to list his tools, explaining, "when you have finished working at the bench you want to eat and smoke a pipe." in other words, he grouped objects according to a concrete situation and not according to the more abstract category "tools." we use the columbia mental maturity scale to test categorical behaviour.. the intelligent boy who, when shown a picture of a hen, a stove, a pot and an egg, says that the egg and the pot belong together, "because you cook an egg in the pot," indicates that he is unable to free himself from the concrete and is not yet ready for classifications. we test the child's ability to give definitions. at the age of six children define objects in terms of function. a six-year-old who says, "you hold it up here," when asked what a violin is, behaves in a very concrete way, which, if persisting, is a poor prognosis in terms of academic functioning. if one asks a number of six-year-old children what a policeman is, one is apt to receive a variety of answers from: "he wears a blue suit, or "he stands at the corner" (which are concrete r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society june responses), to "he directs traffic." in the last response the child tries to cope with function, while in the first one he limits himself to description it is usually the child with a language difficulty who is more concrete than others are. most children who later develop reading disabilities seem to have trouble with patterning the units of words and sentences in spoken speech. orton (12) has shown how frequently these youngsters tend to reverse both oral and printed symbols. the same boy who says "crice ripsies" for "rice crispies" is the one who later on reads "was" for "saw" and "now" for "won." reversal and confusion in the order of sequences — which are closely related to temporal organization in the sense in which it has been discussed — are usually not confined to syllables and words. the whole sentence is often jumbled, showing again that it is in the area of organization and structuralization of both short and long units that children with language deficits have outstanding difficulties. most of our tests are designed to measure the child's ability to pattern, structuralize, and adequately respond to the endless stream of stimuli to which he is exposed at every moment. however, organization of perceptual and motor patterns is not the only area which presents difficulties for our children. many of them have trouble with integration of behavior. the result ifc hyperkinesis and lack of control. most children suffering from developmental language lags are enormously hyperactive. their trouble with inhibition and channeling of impulses seems to be but another aspect of their inability to organize stimuli (arising from inside as well as from outside) into behavioral configurations. hence they find it difficult to sit still several hours a day. such children (they may or may not be emotionally disturbed) are bound to have trouble concentrating. since they are unable to exclude a variety of stimuli, they are incapable of focusing their attention on a specific gestalt, or an assigned task. children in kindergarten are usually given a good many motor outlets, but once they get into first grade they find it difficult to cope with a more structured framework, since their frustration tolerance is low and their need for large muscular activity considerable. among the children whom we have tested during the last few years we have found a fairly steady, though small, number of youngsters whose perceptual deviations, trouble with figure-background relationships, outstandingly poor motor perform' ance and limitation in abstract behavior seem far more severe than is usual for the child who suffers from a developmental language disability. careful investigation of these cases has sometimes revealed a positive history, for instance, anoxia at birth. these youngsters do not necessarily show their usual positive signs on the classical neurological examination. however, more refined testing procedures show that they have difficulties at various levels of integration. watching these children copy the bender gestalt figures, one often finds a marked tendency to disinhibition, accompanied by compensatory rigidity. we find perseveration in various areas and a tendency to go to pieces when the number of stimuli becomes too great. many of these relatively subtle signs go undiscovered until the time when these children are confronted with a task which is as complex as is the mastering of oral and printed symbols. i do not want to give the impression that our testing takes a great deal of time. after some experimenting we have brought the time down to between 40 and 45 minutes. the tests are usually administered during one session, or, preferably, two. careful observation of the child as he functions in kindergarten, moreover, will eliminate many of the more formal procedures. in conclusion i should like to sum up a few points: maturation is largely a process of integration and differentiation. the child of six and older whose perceptual, motor, visuomotor and conceptual performance is still relatively primitive, the child who has trouble with structuralization of behavioral patterns, is the one who is liable to run into difficulties when he is exposed to reading, which requires the smooth interplay of many facets of behaviour. some of these children need more time in which to mature. postponing formal training and discipline for 6 to 12 months may prevent future regrading with its attending experience of failure and humiliation which might easily spoil the youngster's entire learning pattern. but our testing should do more than simply pick out the children who are not as yet ready for first grade. it should assist us in determining what type of help would be suitable for those youngsters who we feel are able to make the grade if given specific assistance and support. the hyperactive child, for instance, needs a teacher who has some tolerance for the child's specific difficulty. he needs, if possible, a setting which allows him a good1 many motor outlets while at the same time providing a somewhat istructured environment which protects him "from an excess of environmental stimuli. such a youngster would fare better if seated in the front of the room where he sees only the teacher and not his classmates. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) june journal of the south african logopedic society the child with an oral language disability, on the other hand, whose lags are confined to specific areas, might do all right if he were referred to a speech therapist and helped to establish more speech patterns and a more extensive vocabulary. another youngster might need assistance with straightening out his confusion in cerebral dominance, and help in establishing left-to-right progression. most of these children, as orton (12) has pointed out, usually do better with a phonetic approach to reading than with the whole-word attack. there are good reasons for this fact and they lie precisely in the direction we have discussed. the child who has trouble with the organization of visual patterns is naturally bewildered and confused if he is confronted with, what to him seem to be diffuse and undifferentiated configurations. he will benefit immensely if words are broken up into small phonetic units. this breaking-up process actually represents a transposal of spatial sequences into temporal ones. in this manner many youngsters are able to cope with single sounds, short auditory configurations which they slowly learn to fuse into larger entities. this procedure facilitates the structuralization of — for them — undifferentiated wholes and thus gives them a larger measure of security. there are, of course, exceptions. there are children who fail to respond to the phonetic method . these particular children fall into three categories: the hyperactive child does not always possess the span of attention required for the laborious sounding out of words. the process is too slow for him and he tends to get discouraged and frustrated. into the second category fall the youngsters who have trouble with abstract behaviour. the process of analyzing a word into its parts and then synthesizing the parts requires a certain level of abstraction. moreover, the very concept that a letter seen represents a speech sound heard is difficult for the brain-injured child (who is usually hyperactive as well) to grasp. in the third category falls the youngster who shows obsessive tendencies. he will stick compulsively to single sounds; he will be too anxious to blend, them successfully into words or to integrate them into meaningful sentences. thus the tests for prediction of future reading disabilities are not only designed to discover the child who is liable to run into trouble with reading, but are also meant to indicate the areas in which a child's performance lags. the tests should actually do more: they should provide a lead as to what specific techniques could be used to advantage in future training. precious time is thus saved, and some children, iat least, are spared the humiliating experience of failure in reading, writing and spelling (which are all important in the earlier grades), a failure which will often carry over into other learning experiences. not all children suffering from potential reading difficulties are primarily emotionally disturbed. however, their basic developmental lag in physiological-psychological functioning makes, them especially susceptible to adverse educational experiences and as a result they often develop secondary emotional difficulties very early.' we hope to discover some of these youngsters before they become educational and emotional casualties. references 1. bender, l. a. a visnal motor gestalt test and its clinical use. american orthopsychiatric assoc, new york, 1938. 2. bender, l., and h. yarnell. an observation nursery. ' am. j. psychiatry, 97:1158, 1941. 3. borell-maisonny, s. les troubles du language dans les dyslexies et les dysortographies. enfance, 5:400, 1951. 4. doll, e. the oseretsky tests of motor proficiency. educational publishers, minneapolis, 1940. 5. gillingham, a. avoiding failure in beading and spelling. independent school bull., nov. 1949. 6. goldstein, k. language and language disturbances. grune & stratton, new york, 1948. 7. goldstein, k., and m. scherer. abstract and concrete behaviour. psychol. monogr., 53:1, 1941. 8. goodenough, f. measurement of intelligence by drawing. world book, yonkers-on-hudson, new york, 1926. 9. head, h. aphasia and kindred disorders of speech, vol. i. cambridge univ. press, london, 1926. mccarthy, d. chap. 9 in manual of child psychology (leonard carmichael, ed.). (2nd ed.) wiley, new york, 1954. 11. mottier, g. tiber untersuchung der sprache lesegestorter kinder. folio phoniatrica, 3: no. 3, 1951. 12. orton, s. t. beading, writing and speech problems in children. norton, new york, 1937. 13. silver, a. diagnostic value of three drawing tests for children. j. pediat, 37:1, 1950. 14. simon, j. contributions a la psychologie de la lecture. enfance, 5:438-447, 1954. 15. stambak, m. le probleme do rhythme dans le developpement de l'enfant et dans les dyslexies devolution. enfance, 5:480-493, 1951. 16. strauss, α., and l. lehtinen. psychopathology and education in the brain-injured child. grune & stratton, new york, 1947. 17. werner, h. towards a general theory of perception. psychol. rev., 59:5, 1952. 18. comparative psychology of mental development. harper, new york, 1940. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) recent research on the parietal lobes, proprioceptive sensibilities and tactile and kinesthetic abilities in speech production ruth m. clark, ph.d.* introduction at the university of denver wc have been trying through research to understand more adequately the part the parietal lobes and proprioceptive sensibilities play in speech production, and to discover if the kinesthetic and tactile abilities of children having "functional" articulatory problems differ from these abilities for normal speaking children. a number of requests have been received asking for data on the above-mentioned researches. the purpose of this article is to review briefly these studies and give references for the benefit of interested colleagues. it is planned that a more adequate report on each study will soon be published, but for the present this overview will give the data requested so those interested can obtain the studies through inter-library loan. it is also hoped that some of the proposed topics listed in the dissertations for further research along these lines will receive attention. three studies will be reviewed in the order in which they were completed. i. the role of the parietal lobes in speech ( 3 ) . importance of the study for years the standard or acceptable approaches for speech training and speech re-training have been the use of sight and hearing. the study under review undertook to determine if, in addition to visual and auditory training, other sensory approaches existed or were feasible to use for speech learning. a study of the opinions of a number of individuals in the speech field, as well as neurologists, indicated that the use of a third sensory avenue, i.e., the stimulation of the tactile and proprioceptive senses, would be another approach to use in speech therapy. the literature in the fields of physiology, neurology and physiological psychology was reviewed t o ' discover the anatomical structures * ruth m . clark (ph.d., university of southern california, 1943) is the director of the s p e e c h clinic and professor of speech, university of denver, denver 10, colorado. which might be used in the teaching of a motor pattern through tactile and/or kinesthetic sensation. in addition to the anatomy of the somesthetic structures, their function was also discussed. in dealing with the "doubly-handicapped" person where the hard-of-hearing individual may miss all or part of the auditory stimulation, or the individual whose sight is impaired may miss most of the visual stimulation, "the urgency of investigating other approaches to speech learning is obvious." (3, p.3). purpose of the study the study was predicated upon the belief that there exists, in addition to visual and auditory avenues, other sensory approaches for use in learning and re-learning speech. the author investigated the possibility of utilizing cutaneous and kinesthetic sensations in the creation, or the improvement of motor patterns for speech. specifically he tried to answer the following questions: 1. what physiological mechanisms does this tactile and/or kinesthetic approach employ? 2. how do these physiological mechanisms function? 3. what cortical receptors receive these tactile and kinesthetic cue messages? 4. how do these cortical receptors function in possibly initiating, terminating, accelerating, or decelerating motor nerve messages destined for the peripheral speech mechanism? 5. how may this "new" sensory approach function in the learning of typical english speech sounds? results the results of the investigation indicated that it is generally believed that numerous exteroceptors and proprioceptors exist in the peripheral speech musculature. it is also believed that somesthetic pathways to the parietal lobe are employed by sensations resulting from touch, pressure and movement stimulation of the exteroceptors, and muscle-stretch and muscle-contraction stimulation of the proprioceptors. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) when the somesthetic spike potential reaches the parietal lobe it appears to possess unique opportunities not available to visual or auditory. spike potentials for initiating, facilitating, or inhibiting the motor movement patterns essential to speech production. (3, p. 116.) three anatomical characteristics of the parietal and frontal lobes provide these "unique opportunities". the author concluded that the nature of these peculiarly adequate sensori-motor connections, "enable the parietal lobes to play a significant role in laying down a cortical mnemonic neutral trace", which facilitates and synchronizes muscle movement patterns essential to speech. it was suggested that for the best results in securing correct motor patterns for speech, auditory and visual stimulations should be integrated with the "significant potential of somesthetic stimulations." (3, p. 149.) ii. the role of proprioceptive sensibilities in speech production (1) importance of study human speech is made possible by proper integration of highly complex neuromuscular reactions of almost the entire organism. in the production of speech, synergy and synthesis of muscular patterns are important. both synergy and synthesis have as their basis proprioceptive sensibilities. "one would be safe in saying that all speech involves muscle activity and all muscle activity involves proprioception". (2, p. 2.) the sensibilities arising from the deeper tissues of the body, principally from the muscles, ligaments, bones, tendons, and joints, are generally considered to be the proprioceptive sensibilities. the study under discussion did not attempt to advance the sense of proprioception to the exclusion of the other senses but rather to point out that the proprioceptive sense is an outstanding supplement to the other senses. purpose of the study according to the author the purpose of this study was two-fold: 1. to discover as nearly as possible the role of proprioception sensibilities in speech production as shown by correlation of information from the areas of physiology, neuro-anatomy, and physical and speech pathology. 2. to attempt to bring together a body of knowledge which will further the understanding of how proprioceptive sensibilities can be utilized in therapeutic procedures. it is suggested that these findings be tested in further studies. (2, p. 9.) results in the individual with a normal central nervous system, proprioceptive control is of such an automatic nature that it is usually given little consideration. "because of this lack of recognition and the apparent subservient status of proprioception to the other sense, which are often considered of greater importance, this sense is neglected in the training programme of those who do not have normal speech". (2, p. 174.) research in the area of physical medicine has shown that these deep sensibilities can be stimulated to enhance the h a b i t a t i o n and rehabilitation of those suffering from neuromuscular disorders. physical therapy has used various modalities successfully in rehabilitation techniques. dr. dittman (2) analyzed these modalities in terms of their effectiveness in setting up a "physical climate for speech", and in terms of their effectiveness in establishing mnemonic memory patterns in the cortex. several proprioceptive therapeutic techniques advanced by persons interested in neuromuscular rehabilitation were also presented. the author also "set up" a proprioceptive therapeutic programme consisting of three stages of progression. the majority of the proprioceptive modalities were considered to be applicable to the speech mechanism per se. however, the tentative conclusions arrived at through the study indicate that more functional research is needed to substantiate the concepts presented. the modalities discussed should supplement the conventional speech therapy techniques, rather than supplant them. iii. a study of lingual sensory characteristics accompanying "functional" disorders of articulation ( 2 ) . importance of the study it is generally conceded that about seventy-five per cent, or more of the speech problems encountered are disorders of -articulation, and therefore occupy the largest part of the speech therapist's load. in the majority of these cases no apparent cause is found for the faulty articulation and they are referred to as "functional". journal of the south african logopedic society june, 1962 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) many important studies have been performed in the area of "functional" articulatory speech problems, but much more needs to be carried on if we are to understand this large group of speech disturbances. research has produced a multiplicity of causal factors, but "conspicuous because of its almost complete absence from research studies is the mention of tactile and/or kinesthetic sense deviations as possible etiological factors". (2, p. 5.) purpose of the study dr. larson (2) wondered if the ability to "feel" existed to the same extent among children with functional articulatory disorders as among children with normal speech. he stated his problem as follows: the purpose of this study was to discover if functional articulatory subjects tend to have different kinesthetic and tactile ability from normal subjects as shown by testing bodily extremities (fingers) and an oral midline area (tongue). (2, p. 1.) procedure. three tests were developed and administered to sixty elementary and junior high school children. the experimental group was composed of thirty subjects with functional articulatory disorders. the control group of thirty was matched on the basis of chronological age, sex, intelligence rating, economic status, and family sibling pattern. the three tests developed were administered to both groups. the tests measured the ability to judge size of objects manually; size of objects orally; and size of small openings with the tip of the tongue. results. the data indicated that neither group showed any consistent superiority over the other when discriminating between sizes of objects manually. the control group excelled the experimental group somewhat in judging between sizes of objects lingually; however, it was not to any acceptable degree of reliability. on the test that compared the two groups on their ability to "discriminate differences between small holes with the tip of the tongue, however, the control group's superiority was decidedly significant beyond the 0 . 1 . per cent, level." (2, p. 116.) the author was extremely cautious in evaluating his results and said, though many children with functional articulatory problems exhibited a lower ability to make fine discriminations with the tongue-tip, no inference was given that tactile deficiency of the tongue-tip was necessarily a fundamental cause of articulatory disorders. "at best, this particular type of deficiency may be considered as one factor within a complexity of etiological patterns". (2, p. 134.) one of the outstanding contributions of this research was the chapter on suggestions for further study and the ingenuity and meticulousness used in devising the tests. references 1. dittman, helen h., "the role of proprioceptive sensibilities in speech production." unpublished doctor's dissertation, university of denver, denver, june, 1955, 231 pp. 2. larson, m.h., "a study of lingual sensory characteristics accompanying 'functional' disorders of articulation". unpublished doctor's dissertation, university of denver, denver, november, 1955, 149pp. 3. tyson, j. d., "the role of the parietal lobes in speech". unpublished doctor's dissertation, university of denver, denver, august, 1954, 149pp. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) march j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society 1 the treatment in a child guidance clinic of emotionally disturbed children showing speech defects by yvonne blake ph.d when a child is referred to a child guidance clinic for a speech defect, the preliminary investigation should be thorough enough to indicate whether the trouble is essentially mechanical (i.e. due to wrong learning habits or to incorrect speech and breathing habits), organic (i.e. a physical defect or deformity affecting either the speech directly or the hearing of sounds) or psychological. obviously, psychotherapy would be essential in the latter cases (if the symptom is serious enough to merit treatment), whereas speech therapy and/or physical help would be necessary for the others. for the purposes of clear argument, it is helpful to assume that the child with organic or mechanical speech defects is psychiatrically healthy and that the psychiatrically ill child can spontaneously correct his speech defects after the emotional disturbance has been dealt with. although this is very often not true, it is a justifiable simplification to make just now. the psychotherapist is thus not a symptom remover; he recognizes the symptoms as an s.o.s. call that justifies a full investigation of the history of the child's emotional development, relative to the environment and to the culture. treatment is directed towards relieving the child of the need to send out the s.o.s. the psychotherapist interested in the dynamics of interpersonal relationships and in dynamic psychology generally, tolerates the symptom and tries to make sense out of it, and to understand the role it plays in the developing human personality. i do not wish to imply that clinics where the avowed aim is to remove the symptom have no value. mothers and children are grateful; and there is nothing to be said against such clinics except that they side-track the whole issue of etiology, of speech defects as a symptom that means something, as an expression of persisting infantile conflict that has value in the economy of the child. in most cases, cure of the symptom does no harm and when a cure could do harm, the child usually manages through unconscious processes either to resist cure or to adopt an alternative s.o.s. sign — one that may produce transfer to another type of clinic. often the symptom is easily seen to be quite a subsidiary phenomenon, a little bit of a huge problem of a human being engaged in trying to develop to maturity in spite of handicaps. it will thus be clear that from the psychotherapist's point of view, it is not initially of major significance whether the child shows through a speech defect or through some other symptom that he is emotionally disturbed, anxious and in need of help. this does not mean that during the course of treatment (often even during the first session) the child will not show why the symptom shown is the particular one of a speech defect rather than some other difficulty; but it does mean that treatment is highly individualized whether the child shows a speech difficulty or some other problem. children who are showing speech defects for emotional reasons cannot be grouped together any more significantly than those showing other disturbances — there may be an emphasis on oral difficulties in such children — particular concern say about oral aggression: biting, eating up, greed — but then again the underlying anxiety or need may be only indirectly concerned with speech or the mouth and more directly with relationships — especially interfamily relationships. i would like to illustrate these points by giving a short account of three cases that were seen by me — all referred for speech defects. t w o of the children were referred for stammering while the third child was described as "talking indistinctly, using jargon and refusing to co-operate with efforts to correct her speech defect." c a s e 1 billy, aged eight, was one of two boys in a good family. his elder brother (three years older than billy) was a straight-forward personality from the beginning and was doing r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society well at school and presented no problem. billy had had a difficult birth, was not breast-fed after the first ten days and had been a restless, difficult baby from the start. he was even now less capable of taking or tolerating frustration, and was described as generally tensed and highly strung. the mother recognized the stammer for which he was referred as " a nervous symptom." at school, billy was thought to be intelligent but restless and his concentration was poor. it seemed from this history that billy's stammer could be associated with the difficulties evident even in infancy; the tense restless attitude had shown itself from very early on and from the mother's description could initially have been associated with the discomfort experienced in the feeding situation, which would tend to confuse the child and stimulate his aggression. billy came very readily to the playroom. he smiled at me and immediately started putting out the small toy figures and animals without waiting for any explanation or introduction. he remarked, with only the trace of a stammer, that he liked coming to the clinic. i suggested that perhaps he did not feel quite sure about coming and felt that i expected him to like it, even though he could not even know at this stage whether i was a likeable person or not. he looked rather surprised and then said with great difficulty and a noticeable blocking in his speech that he had been to two other clinics and "they were no good." i took up with him the fact that he obviously then, far from liking to come here, seriously doubted whether i'd be any good. i explained that i was not concerned with his speech as such, but that i hoped that we could together try to find out what worried him and what interfered with his ability to express himself freely. i pointed out that the toys, the art materials, and everything else in the room could be used freely by him in any way he chose. he started to build a fort, which was filled with wild animals. he said that they were all so fierce that if i was able to see inside the fort i'd run away. he brought the toy gorilla right up to my face saying "look they bite, grrrrr!" he put the toy into his mouth, bared, his teeth, and with the toy still held in his mouth threatened me wih his "grrrr" sounds. i suggested that he was showing me that the fort was himself, that the wild animals were, in fact, inside him, and that he felt that being so full of wild dangerous animal feelings, he had to keep me out for my own protection and keep the wildness inside himself — no wonder it was difficult for him to let out his speech freely because, if he spoke freely the wild animals may come out with the words. "and eat you up," he added. this pre-occupation of billy's with his "inside", effectively interfered with his outside activities; his continual efforts to deal with and control his "wild feelings" made it impossible for him to concentrate on anything else and his restlessness was associated with his efforts "to keep the animals down". these animals were located in his abdomen and there were many rituals and fantasies associated with placating them, trying to tame them, out-witting them and generally preventing them from destroying him and those he loved. as billy gradually worked through his anxieties and realized that his aggression could be used and expressed indirectly (and that even expressing it openly and directly occasionally did not cause complete destruction and over-whelming chaos in the world around him), he was able to integrate his personality more successfully and to accept his aggressive impulses as part of himself. the speech difficulties cleared up without the need for speech therapy; and after about fifteen months of weekly therapy sessions, billy had changed from a confused, disorganized personality who was seriously threatened and disturbed by his difficulties, into a boy who coped very satisfactorily and without undue tension or strain with both his internal and external "worlds". c a s e 2 peter, aged 10. peter was the youngest of four boys. he had had an uneventful infancy, had been a " g o o d " baby and had presented no problems until his fourth year when he developed a stammer. the mother felt that he really was still no problem but the father was very concerned about it, and evidently had been since the stammer first started. as the boy appeared so normal, he had been taken on for speech therapy soon after starting school — he attended a speech clinic for about 18 months and then had speech training at school. these latter sessions were still continuing when he finally came to a child guidance no blocking of speech but simply an audible clinic, but the stammer persisted. there was stammer. during the first few sessions, peter spent most of his time drawing — the drawings represented "patterns" he had done at school and, in fact, revealed very little beyond the fact that he was not wanting to go into his problem. when this was pointed out to him and it was suggested that he perhaps wanted and needed the stammer and that giving it up represented a bigger threat to him than coping with the difficulties it caused him, he denied this strongly and assured me (as he had assured his father and his teachers) that he "would do. anything to be cured". i asked him what, for example, was he willing to do. he thought for a while r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) march journal, o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n logopedic society 13 and then said he would even be willing to let his brother (two years older than himself -— the other two boys were 16 and 18 years respectively and not really important in his life, especially as they were away at boarding school) have his white mice. this led to a discussion of his pets, which the father had given him in the hope that the distraction would help him and cure the stammer. it became obvious that he received many gifts, a considerable amount of attention and enjoyed quite a few privileges from his father — all in the hope of "curing the stammer". the significance of his stammer now became more obvious and also his need not to give it up. as the fourth boy he evidently felt unable to compete directly with his brothers for his father's attention. the stammer was solving this difficulty and bringing him more attention and more material benefits than his brothers enjoyed. at the same time it did cause him considerable suffering and inconvenience, so that he did not have to feel guilty over his need to receive more than his brothers. it was necessary in this case to clarify also to the boy's father how his concern had in a sense encouraged the boy's symptom which seemed to have started as a "normal" speech difficulty of a three year old. as the youngest child, peter had early on felt the,need to compete with his brothers and had shown an over-eagerness to express himself in speech and to make his wants known; and the stammer developed into a problem only when the father became over-anxious about it. t h e father's concern transformed a temporary and very common difficulty into a chronic symptom. this symptom disappeared only when peter could prove to himself, with his father's cooperation, that he could have the attention, love and support he needed without clinging to his symptom. c a s e 3 maisie aged 5. maisie was the elder of two children — her brother being 20 months younger. the mother felt maisie had developed normally in most respects, but had never learnt to talk properly. her developmental quotient and her i.q. were above average and, except for her speech she was coping well. she was not "babyish" in other ways. she simply refused to speak clearly, and if her parents pretended not to understand her demands, unless verbally expressed, she screamed or refused her food. it became evident that to the mother, the most significant factor was the child's refusal of food and not the speech. t o the child it was neither food nor the speech which mattered most but her feeling of having been pushed out by her mother when her brother was born. the mother remembered only later on during treatment that maisie had been bottle fed until the baby was born, and had been abruptly weaned because the mother could not "have two babies" at the same time. maisie deeply resented the deprivation, she resented the attention the brother reccwed and expressed her hostility orally by refusing to "grow up" in this particular way. from these three examples it will be clear that the speech defect was in each case connected with the child's inter-personal relationships. it was only as he was able to sort these out that the need for the symptom disappeared, and development could proceed normally. i do see the point of view of the person, who not being concerned specifically with dynamic psychology, must ignore the meaning of symptoms and must try to cure them. but i do ask these specialists to give the psychotherapist credit for his point of view. the two disciplines should produce different kinds of child-specialists, each with a healthy respect for the other. the emotionally disturbed child needs treatment, which is geared to his specific needs, and which will reveal the meaning of the symptom — be it a speech defect or other difficulty — in the make-up of the child. the symptom is usually an organisation of extreme complexity, one that is produced and maintained because of its value to the developing human personality. the child needs the symptom because of some hitch in emotional development. the psychotherapist is thus concerned with the satisfactory development of the child rather than with the symptom as such. bookbinding r. korytowski 1 mylin court, 24 beit street. doornfontein. telephone 44-9602 four reasons why you should have your books, magazines and thesis bound by us: 1) modern linen in 30 colours. leather with beautiful grains 2) first class craftsmanship guarantee for unbreakable binding 3) quick delivery 4) well known for reasonable prices. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) jequiasaq journal of the south african logopedic society 7 the training of language and conceptual thinking in the child with brain damage by i s a a c jolles south suburban public school co-operative association of cook county hart school, homewood, illinois one of the most perplexing problems to the teacher of educable mentally handicapped children is the pupil who responds readily to the nonacademic program but who fails to make satisfactory growth in the reading activity. this problem confronts the teacher of brain-injured children whose intelligence level is well within the normal range. although this has not been definitely established through medical diagnosis, we have reason to believe 'that the inability of these children to comprehend oral instructions and/or to express their thoughts adequately is the direct result 'of some neurological impairment. before proceeding further with this discussion, a description of the type of child with whom we are concerned in this paper is in order. the most outstanding trait is the marked discrepancy between the iq's obtained on verbal and nonverbal tests of intelligence with the performance on the non-verbal test being superior. for example, in classes for children of normal intelligence one may find a pupil whose stanford-binet (verbal) iq is 85 but whose arthur (non-verbal) iq is 119. in special classes for the educable mentally handicapped one may find a pupil whose standford-binet iq is 69 but. whose arthur iq is 103. in either case the classroom manifestation of this difference in verbal and non-verbal abilities is the same, namely, slow progress in academic work (particularly reading) but quite adequate progress in those activities which do not require dealing with symbols, abstractions, and conceptual thinking. among the mentally deficient this type of child is prevalent enough to have caused the misconception among educators that the mentally handicapped are good with their hands but no good at "book learning." so much of one's intellectual functioning is dependent upon language that it is to be expected that this would b'3 frequently associated with mental retardation. the writer has made a crude study of the effectiveness of the children's primary sensory channels for learning in the classroom (auditory, visual, and kinesthetic.) this study involved an analysis of the children's response to various types of intelligence test items and the correlation of the successes and failures with the three sense modalities mentioned above. the children involved in this study were 69 pupils who were in special classes for the educable mentally handicapped. it was found that the children who presented symptoms of language deficiency were inefficient in their responses to test items involving auditory stimulation whereas those who manifested no such symptoms seemed to be relatively adequate in their responses to items involving auditory stimulation. such a disaovery should not surprise us, for it is obvious (as we have learned from the language development problems of deaf children) that language development depends so much upon a child's responsiveness to the spoken language of others. it should follow, then, that if the child has some neurological impairment which results in an inefficient auditory sensory channel, his language development will be retarded. likewise, his ability to form concepts will be impaired, for conceptual thinking is dependent upon abstract thinking through the use of language symbols. without language one cannot think in abstract terms; without abstraction it is most difficult to form concepts. we in north america learn about africa through some one else's experience, not by going there ourselves. this we can do through language. the child with a serious language deficiency cannot learn about other continents effectively through the experience of others. we should not be surprised, then, when a child of this type replies to the question, "where is chile?", by slaying, "in a can." likewise, he is unable to express the similarity between a plum and a peach, but he probably could see some relationship between them if they were actually within his presence. our problem is how to reach this type of child so that he can learn to think and to read with comprehension, at least within the limits of his intellectual potential. as far as this writer has been able to learn, there are no publications of research that would give information to a teacher on how to train such a pupil to form concepts. therefore, the writer collaborated with miss selma i. southwick, teacher of educable mentally handicapped children in the quincy, illinois, public schools in the development of an r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society instructional approach and a teaching sequence which we believe will b'e useful in achieving our goal. this method has been used by miss southwick during the past two years. in some of the cases progress has been unexpectedly good, in others somewhat slow. at least in most pupils we have observed progress beyond that which we have seen in the past. the approach which · we are using involves intensive training in the forming of concepts. the part that language development plays in this approach will be evident in the description of the teaching sequence which has been broken down into the following steps. 1. teaching the relationship of objects within the immediate experience of the pupil. the normal child accomplishes this step in his development through the use of a readiness work book; he checks objects which differ but in some way are alike and rdlate to each other. this relatively abstract approach (workbook pictures )is adequate for him because of his accurate interpretation of his environment, but in the case of the child with brain damage, be he mentally handicapped or normal in intelligence, this is not the case. he usually has a perceptual disorder which tends to make it difficult for him to interpret his environment accurately. in the usual workbook, he may not check the objects aorrectly; but, if he does check accurately, he cannot explain why he chose as he did. such an achievement is mechanical and cannot be interpreted as an indication of readiness for academic work. before using a workbook, this child needs a prolonged period of training wherein he has actual objects to manipulate. thus, he can feel likenesses and differences in form, in texture and in function; he makes use of his relatively efficient visual and kinesthetic sensory channels. the objects need' to be distinct in color, in form, and in function so that discrimination may be achieved more accurately. the color of the object must be in reasonably sharp contrast from its background. it's size must neither be so large that it completely obliterates the background (thus losing much of its perspective within the environment to the point of losing much of its meaning) nor so small that it becomes lost within the total field. the child needs to verbalize as he manipulates, for by so doing he employs his auditory sense to emphasize what he is experiencing visually and kinesthetically. this is most important in that it helps to develop the effectiveness of his auditory sense modality. verbalizing the experience not only increases the meaning of the experience for the child', but it also offers the teacher an opportunity to develop and to check the accuracy of the child's understanding of the particular situation. for example, in answer to the question: "how is the orange like the apple?" the child with the compliments of gallo (africa) limited distributors of 2)ecca, dsrunsivich, (qallolone, 2). and ^j^oludoi* (records r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e journal of the south african logopedic society may say, "because the orange is an orange." 2. teaching the relationship of pictures of the above objects. the normal child effectively discriminates between the small pictures found in most readiness workbooks. the child with disorders in the language and concept areas may find it more meaningful to sort large, clearly defined pictures, for they help to compensate for his perceptual disability. here again the actual manipulation of the pictures tends to center his attention to a degree that more accurate interpretation is achieved. 3. drill on the relationship of pictures of objects not within the immediate experience of the child. all teachers are well aware of the value of pictures in extending the child's understanding of the world beyond his immediate environment. the normal child looks at the pictures, discriminates, and evaluates the messages presented', particularly as it relates to his past experiences. this is not true for the child with a concept disorder. for example, suppose the teacher presents the following pictures for study: 1. a person lying on a sofa. 2. a person sleeping in a bed. 3. a person sitting quietly under a shady tree. 4. persons playing basketball. 5. a person operating a machine in a factory. the child unable to form good concepts will find it difficult to understand why all the pictures except the last denote some kind of rest or relaxation. this child needs extensive opportunity to study pictures that have very distinct messages. as he discusses the pictures the teacher helps him to clarify his thinking by interjecting questions which help him to work out an accurate interpretation. for example, the child responds with "the orange is an orange," to the question stated above. the teacher then directs his thinking as follows: "yes, an orange is an orange. is an orange round like :a ball? is an apple round like a ball? can you eat an orange? can you eat an apple? can you eat a ball?" the teacher will note the similarity of this exercise to exercises in the traditional workbook. there are, however, important differences for the child we are discussing. this child must have concrete experiences in order to observe similarity, i.e., actual fruits if they are available, sharply defined pictures as a second best visual aid. the child works out his reasoning orally so that he hears his sequence of ideas which give him the correct concept regarding the apple and the orange. 4. teaching to transfer this type of thinking to other situations. 5. teaching the relationship between words, pictures and objects with stress being placed on pretoria school for cerebral palsy p.o. box 1511, pretoria. applications are invited for the post of speech therapist salary: (4 year course) £600 χ 30 — £960 four school holidays annually. to assume duties as soon as possible. apply: the principal, p.o. box 1511, pretoria. the meaning of words. 6. drill on the forming of concepts with words ivithout the aid of objects and pictures. 7. redrill on the transfer of this training to other situations. steps 4 through 7 of the developmental sequence follow the methods suggested above for steps 1 through 3. each problem which the child confronts must require a choice of solutions with the necessary reasoning involved in making the choice. for example, phonetic instruction in reading is of little value unless the child is required to transfer the learning of the drill period to actual reading situations that are meaningful to him. if he has learned the short sound of "a", then he should have to face an unknown word which is controlled by this sound so that the teacher will see that he applies his new learning correctly in order to help him pronounce this word. in conclusion, the type of child whom we have been discussing here must start slowly with a simple experience which is so very concrete that he can see it, hear it, manipulate it to the degree necessary to prove to himself its actual reality. for this reason we must adhere to the sequence mentioned above in the training of concepts as well as in the development of language itself. in other words, we start with the concrete objects, go on to pictures, then to the word, and finally to the printed word which is the ultimate in symbolism. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y speech recording in stuttering therapy erica stern, b.a., dip. log. south africans generally, and school children in particular, have become increasingly radio-conscious within recent years. no doubt the advent of "springbok radio" has had a great deal to do with this. after school is over for the day, children enjoy listeningin to tales of adventure and travel, radio plays and dramatised broadcasts of stories, a s well a s a variety of other programmes. thus the pupil who is given the opportunity of making a recording of his speech is bound to win the admiration of his listeners when the recording is played back, since a tape (or wire) recorder is still a novelty, not being yet in everyday use. however, if a stuttering pupil is to derive enjoyment and benefit from the use of the recorder, certain considerations should be borne in mind by the speech therapist. firstly, children who are asked to speak impromptu into the microphone of a recording instrument are likely to be more hesitant in their speech than is usually the case. this will become apparent when the recording is played back. secondly, children are likely to be adversely affected upon hearing a recording of their own, non-fluent speech. the above points are illustrated by the case of jimmy, mentioned by prof. wendell johns o n . ( 2 ) it is the opinion of the writer, therefore, that a tape recorder should only b e used with stuttering pupils when it is possible to record a fluent sample of their speech, or at least speech that is decidedly more fluent than their usual way of speaking. hearing such a recording of. their speech will give a feeling of encouragement and satisfaction to the stutterers. | various recordings by university ! students. lucile cypreansen ( 1 ) reports interesting experimentation in the making of different types of recordings with a group of 14 university of nebraska men students (all of whom were stutterers), between the a g e s of 18 and 23 years, a s part of the speech -therapy programme during the first course of fifteen meetings. speech recordings were made from the 11 th to 14th meetings. 11th meeting. "each student tried the ' two-room' technique, speaking extemporaneously and reading aloud while shut off in a room by himself. in a connecting room, closed off by a door, the supervisor made electrical transcriptions of what w a s being said. the microphone of the recorder w a s placed in the room with the speaker. interesting results were noted. the speaker knew the recording w a s going on, but he did not know just when his speech was being recorded. speech patterns on the records showed much improvement over speech patterns used in daily speech. the subjects were pleased and encouraged by the results of the recordings. 12th meeting. "a type of 1 p s y c h o d r a m a ' was tried. the students were given a hypothetical incident to dramatise informally without previous practice. "two students were placed in the room with the microphone. they proceeded to carry on the dramatisation of the incident. there w a s very little stuttering noticed in this type of dramatisation. the supervisor and the rest of the group in the other room could hear over the connecting amplification system all that w a s said. parts of the dramatisations were recorded. later the recordings were played back to the group. the subjects were free and easy in this project and appeared to enjoy the performances thoroughly . . . "an assignment w a s m a d e for the next meeting. each student w a s to bring a threeor four-minute selection that he would like to read to someone else. 13 th meeting. "the selections assigned for oral reading were practised on the wire recorder. two students worked together on one recorder, recording and listening, and offering suggestions to each other . . . 14th meeting. "electrical transcriptions were made of readings of the material which had been previously assigned and practised on the wire recorder. the members of the group were allowed to take their records home to play for their families and friends. they were then returned and filed in the individual clinic folders." cypreansen reports that by the next (15th) meeting (which w a s the end of the first r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y o c t o b e r course), "most of the members of the group appeared to have gained greater confidence in speaking situations . . . and several of the students appeared to show definite improvement in speaking habits." at the opening of the following term the students decided to continue with their group work. additional projects were undertaken and "the making of the recordings was to be continued." recorded programmes by primary school children. in the larger centres of the c a p e province, where classes for speech defective school children a r e carried on under the c a p e education department, the members of the itinerant staff have at their disposal a tape recorder, and a couple of reels for the use of each staff member. up to the present time, however', the writer has had only limited experience in the use of the tape recorder for dramatisation with stuttering pupils. the tape recorder offers some advantages over the presentation of "live" plays. (a) it is preferable to use the recorder in cases where the speech of the stuttering pupil has begun to show an improvement, but where the therapist fears that there is a possibility of the pupil's speech breaking down in front of an audience. (b) there is bound to be an improvement in the speech of the pupils after a number of rehearsals. "the adaptation effect (the tendency for stuttering to decrease with successive readings of the same passages) w a s first reported in 1937 by johnson and knott, and it has since been repeatedly confirmed in experimental studies . . . johnson and innes found that in five successive readings of a short p a s s a g e there was an a v e r a g e reduction in frequency of stuttering of approximately 50 per cent."® (c) if desired, the therapist can make more than one recording of the same programme, and select the best (or better one) to be played back. (the others can b e erased and that portion of the reel used again). (d) certain radio plays and broadcast programmes can be recorded easily which would be difficult to present on a stage. the recordings were made by 13 boys, whose speech was marked by stuttering in varying degrees, from standards 1 to 5 inclusive in primary schools in cape town. these pupils were receiving treatment in three groups of three each, and two groups, each consisting of two pupils. the four latter pupils were combined into one group for the purpose of the recording. the pupils were told that they could make a recording to be played back to their classes at the end of the second quarter (in june). one of the groups (consisting of three standard 5 pupils), preferred to write their own programme. thus "tintown calling," (a skit on "snoektown calling"), came into being. it incorporated the pupils' own ideas, with some help from the therapist. the pupils were particularly keen on sound effects, and brought tins, a cane and a knobkerrie for the purpose. for use with stuttering pupils (whom the writer prefers to treat in small groups of two or three), very suitable, graded plays are to be found in harrap's "new dramatic readers," books 1—4, and in the recently published series of five books entitled "prettige praatstories" (nasionale boekhandel). the three plays to be recorded by the groups were chosen from the above books by those taking part. each pupil was asked which part he would like to take. if only one pupil chose a particular part, he was given that part in the play. otherwise lots were drawn for the parts. thus no pupil felt that any favouritism had been shown, and all were afterwards perfectly satisfied with their parts. a typewritten script was given to each pupil to enable him to rehearse the play at home with his parents, brothers and sisters. the class teachers concerned showed keen interest in the idea and were eager to make the necessary arrangements for the performances (i.e., the playing of the recordings to the different classes). all the performances achieved great success. the listeners showed their appreciation by requesting, "can't we hear it again," and it w a s obvious that the stutterers, who were able to observe the reactions of the audience, derived great enjoyment from hearing the recordings they had made. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y since rehearsals for the recordings were held for a comparatively short period of time, (owing to the illness of the therapist), and a s this method was only used a s part of the treatment that the pupils received, it is difficult to estimate the degree of improvement that the use of this technique brought about in the speech of . the pupils (if at all), and for how long it would have lasted. the writer would b e interested to know of the results obtained by other speech therapists using speech recording in stuttering therapy. bibliography. 1. cyprecmsen, lucile, "group therapy for adult stutterers," the journal of s p e e c h a n d hearing disorders, december, 1948, p a g e s 313-319. 2. johnson, wendell, "speech handicapped school children," harper a n d brothers, newyork, 1948. appendix. 3. johnson, darley and spriestersbach, "diagnostic manual in speech correction," harper a n d brothers, new york, 1952, p a g e 122. p.o. box 10482 telephones 22-4656 23-9432 abe etkind (pty.) ltd. wholesale merchants. first floor, lincoln house, 57 pritchard street, tohcamesburg. world's finest hearing-aids. 3 transistor from £29/15/0 wd the (^omplimentd 4 specialist suppliers of physiotherapy and rehabilitation equipment telephone 23-8106 236, jeppe street, johannesburg p.o. b o x 3 3 7 8 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) cleft lip and palate by d. h. walker. f.r.c.s.(ed.) plastic surgeon, johannesburg hospital and university of the witwatersrand. at least one in every thousand babies born will have a hare lip or cleft palate, or both. to have some idea of how to treat and repair these is not enough; the immediate method is of little value unless one tries to assess its possible effects on the life and behaviour of a young adult twenty years hence. there is a quotation attributed to professor t. p. kilner and used by sir harold gillies which epitomises the ideal end result — "they shall look well, speak well and eat well." it is unlikely that any one person can acquire and use all the skill, judgement and art needed to approach this ideal end-result. the methods used at present are in the hands of three groups of people:— (1) plastic surgeons and nurses. (2) orthodontists. (3) speech therapists. these are not given in any order of importance or priority but rather in a possible order of consultation. each is important in his or her own field and at a particular time — in the progress of treatment, any member may have top priority in helping the patient. it is our practice, whenever possible, to see the baby with cleft lip and/or palate on the day of birth or soon after. once a probable treatment plan has been laid down, the parents can be told of all the positive help that can be given to their son or daughter. it is known that about 20% of all cleft lip and palate patients have a family history of this anomaly; this should be recorded and constantly kept in mind. the help and advice of a skilled geneticist may be sought if a larger family is to be planned with reasonable safety in this group. of the remaining 80%, a large proportion is associated with poor living conditions or malnutrition — either immediate or in the past. the usual speculative factors of virus and other infections are quoted. however, a strong case may be deduced for the grave dangers of the use of steroids during the first three months of pregnancy. these substances need very special indications for their use and should probably be ordered for a pregnant woman only by doctors in consultation. all classifications are man-made and usually false in many respects. rigid classifications of natural phenomena are perhaps as good an indictment as any of the great mental trap of mankind — man's pathetic and frequent belief in the infallibility of his own reasoning. different centres and different countries use varying, variable and often conflicting classifications. it is suggested that a brief accurate description is one of the best forms cf classification. cleft or hare lip. (a) unilateral or bilateral. (b) incomplete or complete. cleft palate. (a) complete or incomplete. (the latter sometimes referred to as a posterior cleft of varying degree). (b) unilateral or bilateral. for example. hendrik v.d. merwe. unilateral complete cleft lip (l). palate intact, notched alveolus. john smith. bilateral cleft lip and palate. (l) lip cleft: incomplete. (r) lip cleft: complete. permutations of these allow for all but the rare anomalies. the extra writing of a few words is more than worth the extra clarity of the mental picture produced for any one who is at all familiar with the appearance of children with clefts. there is little, if any, room for the concept of repair of a cleft lip or palate as an december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) emergency procedure. when this form of request is made it generally relates to feeding difficulties. these can nearly always be overcome by (a) breastfeeding the baby as much as possible to encourage a normal sucking reflex, (b) completing the extra requirements by using a feeding bottle with a suitable teat. there are many examples of children growing to be well developed and articulate adults without any surgical procedures or any other treatment of their cleft lips and palates. one has recently seen an adult male with an untreated bilateral cleft lip and palate who is eighty-five years old. a further patient with not so severe a disability is a woman over sixty years old who has an untreated, wide cleft palate extending the whole length from the post-alveolar region to the uvula. this woman has perfectly intelligible speech and, although when young had difficulty in preventing certain foods and fluids from escaping via her nose, now has learnt to control this abnormality completely and eats and drinks in an exemplary manner. she has three female grandchildren, all of whom have identical clefts to her own, and informs us that this is a common abnormality in all female members of her family but is not found in the males. the eldest of the three grandchildren has already had her palate repaired and now rejoices in normal speech. to return to the question of early operation: one does occasionally repair, under local anaesthesia in the maternity hospital, a partial cleft lip of moderate degree — generally when the palate is intact and the lip presents the only defect. the patient should be about a week old and in excellent health. our more usual procedure is to ask the orthodontist to see a child with cleft lip and/ or palate in the maternity hospital during the b first week of life. it is possible with modern techniques, skill and materials to take dental impressions at this early stage and to make an accurately fitting prosthesis or obturator which closes at least the front of the cleft in the gum and palate completely. this is held in position by a light headcap which is attached by rubber bands to smooth, light, curved wires which project to the exterior from the small plate in the baby's mouth. with a little patient, gentle and persistent replacement the baby soon accepts the appliance and feeds quite readily with it in position. the object of this is to produce a better, more even alignment of the parts of the alveolus on either side of the cleft. not only will this, after a few months, make the operation easier and more successful, but it will improve the external contour and, internally, pave the way for a better palate repair. the success of such an appliance is largely contributed to by the child, since persistent palatal and facial movements such as occur in sucking, perform a strong and almost continuous moulding action, transmitted by the orthodontic appliance. it has been found, however, that there may be a rapid decrease in the degree of spectacular improvement if the method is not started in the first three months of life. after many years of argument and evidence, world authorities are still undecided as to the best age for surgical treatment. a general method for cleft lip is to choose any age over three months, or when the child weighs about twelve pounds or more and is in good health and gaining weight steadily. it is preferable for the child to be on a mixed diet. the presence of early erruption of teeth is not a bar to any of the usual standard operations. some teeth, badly out of position — "ectopic teeth" as they are called, may be safely removed at the operation for repair of the lip. the essentials of this operation are best considered in the form of priciples — there are dozens of technical variations in actual operative procedure — all aiming at the same end-result: a lip which on close inspection looks as though the patient once had an accident in the form of a cut lip (and possibly cut nostril floor) which has left a faint scar. the important points to look for are:— (i) that the nostril bases are on the same horizontal line and the nostril floors — from nostril margin to the septum — are of the same width. (ii) that the sides of the cleft are of the same length and hence the depth of lip below each nostril is the same on both sides. 6 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) (iii) that the muco-cutaneous junction (i.e. the junction of the skin and red margin) form a smooth continuous line, without "steps" and with the same depth of mucous membrane showing on both sides. there may or may not be a "cupid's bow". a large number of surgeons' names are attached to the operations described for repairing hare lips — all aim at the results described. some of the more common names are mirault, blair, kilner, rose, le mesurier, veau, tennison, and, more recently, millard. it is, in the end result, most important to avoid a "tight lip" — stretched from side to side. incidentally, a device preventing unreasonable side pull on recently inserted stitches is called a "logan's bow" and consists of a smooth u-shaped piece of metal fastened to the cheek by adhesive strapping so that it rests astride the newly repaired lip and relaxes the margins of the cleft. stitches remain in for a varying period, depending on tension and other factors, but most are out within a week. the patient is usually in hospital for about ten days. whenever possible some part of the gap in the gum and the nostril floor are repaired at the same time as the lip. a suitable age for cleft palate surgery is often considered to be between one and two years. at this age the child is beginning to make sounds which have a definite meaning and, as the end of the second year approaches to say simple words in imitation of adults and older children. surgical repair at this stage is done partly on the basis of the theory that learning to speak with a closed palate (rather than an open, unrepaired one) will produce fewer bad speech habits and their subsequent imprinting on the child's mind will be minimal, and partly to aid in normal eating. against repair at this time are the findings of a number of workers — including those engaged in a long term survey for the american orthodontic association. follow ups of the order'of twenty years have been done on patients who had radical repairs of cleft palates in the first year or two of life. many of these operations featured a very radical separation of soft parts from bone in the preceding lip repair as well as in the later palate repair. this usually takes the form of elevating as much of the lining as possible of both nasal and oral surfaces of the bony palatal shelves, so that soft parts may be moved towards the midline to close the cleft, and also displaced backwards to give length and mobility to the repaired palate. it is believed that this may, in some cases contribute towards severely retarded development of the middle third of the face, becoming most noticeable as adult life is approached. this is not, of course, conclusive evidence that the radical nature of the operation in the cases investigated was the direct and entire cause of the mal-development of the face and the palate. firstly the term "radical" must be qualified by knowing exactly the extent and technical nature of the dissection at the operation. secondly, the abnormality of a cleft palate may include anomalies of the blood supply and irregularities in the growth centres of the bones of the face — in number, position or behaviour — or in combinations of any of these. of major importance in the growth of the centre of the face in a forward direction, is the nasal septum, based in turn on the vomer, a bone which resembles a vertical partition, rising high in the naso-pharynx from the base of the skull. interference with the blood supply of the vomer may result in failure of the tip of the nose and upper lip to grow forward sufficiently — hence the occasional flat profile of the repaired upper lip. this deformity is sometimes contributed to by a slow inward collapse of the alveolar arches with a final contour that is small and pointed towards the front. as the information just outlined was collected it became important to know when, if the age of two years is too early, it might be reasonably safe to operate on a growing face in a relatively radical manner. it is now suggested that sufficient growth of the middle region of the facial skeleton has taken place by about the age of four years to minimize the risk of serious facial deformity after operation. nevertheless one should remember that there is no finality in this matter and the exact age for operation is often varied for different patients. if the child is intelligent enough to try and december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) use recognisable words before operation there should be a period of meetings with the speech therapist, frequent enough to develop friendly contact and confidence. this should culminate in a careful, unhurried tape recording and pre-operative speech assessment. general health before operation must be good and examination should show the child to be free of any other gross abnormality. it is not uncommon for congenital defects to occur together in the same patient, e.g. an abnormal heart, a hernia or a genito-urinary abnormality. these, or any other defects, do not as a rule prevent successful treatment of cleft lip and palate. from a plastic surgeon's point of view, one aim stands head and shoulders above the rest — normal speech. in order to promote the highest standards of work this aim should be taken literally. the best results should produce speech indistinguishable from that accepted in persons who have no cleft palate. it is believed that the anatomical and physiological factors contributing to normal speech include an active closing off of the oro-pharynx from the naso-pharynx while speaking. the main aid to this is probably a long, mobile soft palate capable of brisk, symmetrical muscle contraction. as he commences the operation by mobilising the soft parts and testing their displacement backwards and towards the mid-line, this thought must be uppermost in the surgeon's mind. as the dissection progresses each step should lead to a repair which consists, if possible, of a complete closure, using the patient's tissue in such a way that no artificial device will be needed later and no fistulae will remain or develop. at most, one's compromise on this arrangement should consist of an occasional case with a temporary obturator and the intention of planned fistula closure later. the application of these principles is made clearer by reference to the common types of cleft (the simplest first) and very brief descriptions of the operations commonly used in their repair. a. cleft soft palate only. sometimes referred to as a "posterior cleft" this may vary from a bifid uvula to the whole length of the soft palate ending in a v-shaped notch in the back edge of the bony hard palate. this latter deformity can be felt by gently putting a finger in the child's mouth. the older operation for the repair of this is associated with the name of langenbeck and consists of excising the edges of the cleft facing each other and thus displaying two layers of mucous membrane in a "sandwich-formation" with muscle as the filling on each side of the cleft. next a long incision is made in the soft and hard palate on the inner side of the teeth of the upper jaw. the extent of these incisions, on each side, is from the hamulus to the level of the canine tooth. the soft parts on either side of the cleft are then lifted cleanly off the bone and the greater palatine artery is carefully exposed on the underside of each strap-like flap. if the two strap-like flaps — attached back and front — have been sufficiently mobilised and the cleft is not too wide, they may be sewn together in the midline with two layers of stitches — one on the nasal side and one on the mouth side with the knots facing down into the mouth from the newly constituted roof. one of these layers of stitches should include muscle as it passes through the edges. if the cleft is wide and the greater palatine artery acts as a tethering mechanism under the flap, this may be divided without serious complications. what does produce serious complications is suturing under tension — this must be avoided. the disadvantages of the langenbeck operation are that it does not guarantee permanent lengthening of the palate as the raw undersurfaces of the flaps may scar, shrink and contract in the weeks and months following the operation and so pull forward the uvula and back edge of the soft palate. this minimises the chances of good closing off of the naso-pharynx and hence makes normal speech more difficult. it is, in any case, difficult to be sure of what one has achieved oil the speech side for at least three months after operation and sometimes not for a year or more. the name of victor veau, a french surjournal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) geon is sometimes associated with the operation which uses two long flaps in repair of a cleft palate, at first resembling von langenbeck's flaps, but dividing the front attachments of these flaps so that they are only living on their attachments to the soft palate. backward displacement and suturing in the mid-line are much easier but care must be taken to avoid the formation of a fistula towards the front of these flaps. a similar operation, but using two shorter flaps and permitting division of the greater palatine arteries with less risk of devitalising any part of the flaps, is known as the 'wardill operation'. this method described by wardill of newcastle, is sometimes called the ύ-υ' procedure since the shape of the incision at first outlines a v and then as the flaps slide backwards, come more to resemble a y. occasionally in a narrow cleft where the palate is already long, and favourable in other respects, the edges of the cleft may be paired as previously described and joined by stitches in the mid-line with little or no relaxing or releasing at the sides. carefully planned repairs of soft palate clefts only, should yield between eighty and one hundred percent normal speech. b. cleft soft palate and a variable amount of hard palate. — e.g. two-thirds or three-quarters cleft but still with the alveolus intact i.e. no 'gap in the gum'. the vomer, covered with mucous membrane, is normally well exposed and one can look into the naso-pharynx through the cleft in the palate. it is still possible to use the langenbeck operation for these clefts but one of the best procedures is the so called 'four flap' operation of wardill which uses two posteriorly based flaps for the main part of the repair and two smaller flaps based on the front of the hard palate, behind the teeth, to close tlhe extreme front of the cleft and avoid a fistula in so doing. cuthbert has also designed an operation for this type of cleft . . . particularly for use when the| vault of the palate is high and changes its direction suddenly as the cleft is traced from front to back. this operation is of the three-flap variety and avoids the risk of fistula . . . particularly at the hard/soft junction— an area notorious for fistula formation following a progressive build up of tension while stitches are being put in. cuthbert's operation consists of using a long flap from one side and dividing the other side of the cleft into two flaps, one based on the front and the other on the back. the long flap is made to follow a curved course across the cleft, from side to side and in doing so crosses in between the two short flaps which are stitched to it. this gives a very long, mobile repair and minimises very greatly the effects of any shrinkage or contracture from back to front. c. complete clefts . . . unilateral or bilateral . . . usually associated with a cleft lip as well. there is, of course, a gap in the gum and a cleft lip is generally present as well. an approach originally described by dorrance, has several advantages in dealing with the difficult region at the front of the hard palate. the method may apply to complete, unilateral and bilateral clefts. a flap of mucosa from the vomer (or flaps from both sides of this partition in a bilateral cleft) is mobilised and its free edge moved across to be tucked under a raw edge prepared in the un-united margin of the hard palate part of the cleft. when this has healed, almost the whole of the hard palate has been closed by the use of nasal lining only. the remaining part of the problem now resembles the closure of a cleft which has involved the soft palate alone. factors influencing the success of surgery are usually those of timing and technique. timing in the sense of operating in the optimum period, includes consideration of general health and weight in relation to age. timing must give consideration to the orthodontist — he must have started work early enough and have had long enough to have achieved maximum moulding before superadded surgical movement of soft tissues is undertaken. the speech therapist's orientation to timing must include the problem of whether or not the patient's operation has been delayed for so long that bad speech habits are in danger of becoming heavily imprinted on the brain. there may be danger from previous inexpert speech treatment elsewhere. no amount of palate and lip closing, howdecember, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ever expertly done, can rectify serious mental blocks to normal speech. such cases call for the highest degree of understanding and skill in a speech therapist. as a generalisation it seems reasonable to say that fewer children with early adequate repairs of cleft palates need speech therapy than do those whose repairs are delayed until a fair-sized collection of badly pronounced words is in daily use. faulty surgical techniques are often perpetuated by operators who have been repairing cleft palates for many years and acquired experience without wisdom. the surgeon who is to achieve a high standard of results in his work, must be ruthless in his criticism of himself, and whenever an operation is unsuccessful, look first to himself and much later to the patient or instruments or type of operation for the cause of failure. briefly the surgeon must strive for a long mobile palate closed without perceptible tension, without rough handling of muscle and too severe an interference with blood supply. the swelling following the operation produces a marked degree of stiffness of the palate. this may persist in some mild form for weeks or months. for this reason one frequently suggests that no speech therapy should be started for about three months and during this time attention should not be drawn to the child's speech. the child should be allowed to make attempts to speak in the same way as any normal child — by imitation of the parents and older children. whenever the parents are able to show an appreciation of the principles underlying treatment these should be explained to them. the child himself, should never if possible, be allowed to feel that he is any different from other children. if and when any embarrassing situation arises — usually by a reference to the lip scar or speech peculiarities — the child should be encouraged to reply at once, if he is old enough, saying that he has had an operation and is getting better. several residual problems arise — some in the form of questions from speech therapists and parents. one of these problems is the occasional doubt as to whether speech therapy should be given in the presence of a fistula — can it be harmful in such cases? the answer to this may depend on the site of the fistula and the intensity of therapy. intensive efforts to master the technique of using the tongue to close off an anterior fistula, while still trying to produce normal speech with the rest of the palate and tongue, may produce a confused state of mind about certain sounds if an obturator is inserted later, or the fistula closed by a further operation. if an early operation or closure by obturator is not possible, it may be better to ignore wrong sounds produced by an anterior defect — if the rest of the anatomy is functioning reasonably well. a fistula further back — the common site in many repairs is the junction of hard and soft palates — may not need any special therapy at all since its closure may be effected by ordinary movements of the tongue and by muscle movements of the palate in which it exists. incidentally, it is most unlikely that speech therapy, if started more than two weeks after a successful operation, would ever cause breakdown of a surgical repair. removal of tonsils and adenoids in a cleft palate patient is usually a factor against normal speech rather than for it. unless the evidence for the removal of tonsils is very strong this operation should be avoided. if it has to be done this should be at least six to twelve months after the repair of the palate. the tonsils should be removed with special care and gentleness by an ear nose and throat surgeon who is familiar with the factors responsible for normal palate function in speech. great efforts should be made to retain the adenoids as they may play a vital part in avoiding nasal escape. plastic surgeons are often asked to predict the chances of normal speech. many clinics claim eighty per cent of normal speech after cleft palate repairs. there is probably more / 7 of the exceptional than the usual in this figure. it is probably safe to say that, with a child who is mentally normal, repairs of clefts of the soft palate only should yield normal speech in over eighty per cent of cases; repair of other clefts should yield normal speech in over fifty per cent of cases. post operative examination of the patient should not lead to judgements on the grounds 1 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) of appearance of the patient's repaired palate. an ugly asymmetrical repair may be found in a patient with completely normal speech, and a palate that looks very close to the normal anatomy may produce poor speech. the function is more important than the apparent structure. to return briefly to the question of obturators: can they ever do harm? it depends on what one means by "harm". a mechanically unsound obturator may cause dangerous ulceration by friction and pressure. a loose obturator or one which is too small because of the rate of growth of the patient may cause peculiar defects and the rest of the child's speech may be harmed by strenuous struggles by parents and speech therapists when all that is needed is a new obturator. this point should be checked repeatedly as the child grows. one 'type of obturator worthy of special mention is that which is made with a very thick centre section, protruding downwards towards the middle third of the tongue. this sometimes has the effect of forcing the patient to move the tongue away to produce certain sounds adequately. if the design is correct, the tongue may be forced backwards and a more effective naso-pharyngeal closure produced — resulting in reduced nasal escape. mention should be made of the problem of sub-mucous clefts. these are not rare and prove well the point that more than mere closing, in the mechanical sense, of a hole or a cleft is required for good or normal speech. a patient with a sub-mucous cleft has cleft palate speech, yet when his mouth is opened the palate at first appears intact. a strong light will reveal that the centre line of the palate — particularly the soft palate — has the appearance of a thin greyish strip. this area consists of two layers of lining only (oral and( nasal) with no muscle in between to complete the sandwich. when an attempt is made to speak or produce a vowel sound the muscle either side of this thin |area pulls upwards and backwards, but also 'outwards towards the side wall of the pharynx. normal closure is not possible and cleft; palate speech results. there can hardly be a better illustration of the importance of a functioning palate as opposed to a palate which is merely closed. december, 1962 median cleft lip — upper or lower — is so rare that it barely deserves a mention. it is amenable to treatment by the application of standard principles. cleft palates are associated with several other conditions, two of which are not uncommon: 1. the pierre robin syndrome. 2. acrocephalosyndactyly. 1. the pierre robin syndrome. this patient has difficulty in feeding; choking and going blue from birth. on examination the lower jaw is small and very far back. so is the tongue, so that its muscle mass may rise up into the naso-pharynx, block this cavity (partly) and orally the entrance to the larynx. there is a cleft soft palate. normal co-ordination between breathing and swallowing is not present and fluid may enter the child's lungs. this is dangerous and may prove fatal. early treatment in hospital is essential, and there may be a case for early use of obturators, or even the rare method of an early closure of the cleft palate which may force the tongue to assume a more normal position. if all goes well the lower jaw should grow forward during the early years of life. this deformity is typified by the description of it as "andy gumpism" or "the chinless wonder". 2. acrocephalosyndactyly. this is the label given to a child born with a tall skull, flattened from back to front, with a posterior cleft palate and fused fingers and toes. little has been said about the intelligence of the patient. this is not because it is unimporant but because little is known of its significance. individuals vary greatly, but there is reason to believe that the more intelligent the patient the more subtle can be the teaching. there is a good deal of resemblance to some of the problems of hand surgery. here too, the better the intelligence of the person, the more likely one is to achieve subtle movements. so it is with the palate and its fine structure — linked strongly to the brain above, inviting us never to be satisfied nor to lack criticism of our own efforts as long as there is the chance of giving our patient contact with his fellows by means of normal speech. journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society ι r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) summary it is at present believed that one person alone cannot carry out the best possible treatment of the patient with a cleft lip and palate. a good team consists of three groups:— 1. plastic surgeons and nurses. 2. orthodontists. 3. speech therapists. about 20% of all cleft lip and palate patients have a family history of this condition. of the remaining 80% a large proportion are born into families in which malnutrition is prevalent. the role of virus and other infections is uncertain. until more is known of the effect of steroids, these should probably only be given to pregnant women with the greatest caution and fullest consultation — particularly in the first three months of pregnancy. an average overall incidence of the deformity is still about one in a thousand births. an initial treatment plan may be made in the maternity hospital. the orthodontist can begin moulding the alveolar contour with a small splint attached by elastic bands to a webbing headcap. subsequent operations are generally easier and more successful. for classification prior to operation, it may be clearer to substitute a brief standard description rather than the ordinary textbook labels. hare-lip and cleft palate operations are rarely, if ever, emergencies — patients with even the severest forms of clefts can reach an otherwise healthy adult state with little or no treatment — although this should never happen in modern surroundings. there is no one age believed to be the best for either lip or palate repair — world opinion still differs on this subject. a general plan suggests that good results follow lip repair after the age of about three months and palate repair before the age of two years. there is some evidence for less interference with growth in the middle third of the face if the palate operation is less radical and/or delayed until the age of four years. lip repair aims at a fine white scar with nostril floors on the same horizontal level and the nostril base at the same distance from the mid-line on each side of the columella. the red margin should form a smooth line from right to left without any "steps" and with the same depth of mucous membrane on either side: the sides of the cleft should be of equal length. the patient is usually in hospital for about ten days. the prime aim of cleft palate repair is normal speech. it is believed that this is approached by a repair which produces a long, mobile soft palate with good muscular union. the soft palate should play a major role in closing off the oro-pharynx from the nasopharynx during speech. for cleft soft palate only, the langenbeck operation is not as satisfactory as the v-y or wardill type operation. for more extensive clefts, but with the alveolus still intact, use may be made of the wardill "four-flap" operation with a contribution from the mucous membrane of the vomer or septum. one of the best methods of ensuring a really long palate with minimal risk of a fistula, is the cuthbert "three-flap" operation. the two posterior flaps are of unequal length and the hard palate repair includes a short anteriorly based flap. repair of complete clefts, unilateral or bilateral, may include extensive use of the mucous membrane on either side of the vomer as a first stage. if this maneouvre is successful, the operation for the second stage in a few months time is reduced to the closure of the soft palate only. factors influencing the success of surgery are mostly matters of timing and technique — provided the patient is correctly . assessed and is fit for operation. timing and / technique are important in the work of all three groups mentioned at the outset. generally fewer children need speech therapy if repairs are early and adequate. the child whose operation is delayed long enough to allow the accumulation, of a .fair sized collection of badly pronounced words will almost always have difficulty in attaining normal speech. 12 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) whenever the parents are able to show an appreciation of the principles underlying treatment, these should be explained to them. the start of speech therapy may safely be delayed for weeks or even up to three months if swelling and scarring in the newly repaired palate take a long time to settle. at this time it may be well to allow he child to try and speak without correction — merely allowing the normal method of learning by imitation of adults and older children. speech therapy is rarely likely to be harmful at any stage. a possible hazard is intensive training in using the tongue to develop and control sounds in the presence of an anterior fistula while still trying to produce normal speech with the rest of the tongue and palate. it is unlikely that speech therapy started more than two weeks after operation could cause damage. tonsils should only be removed, in cleft palate cases, by an operator with knowledge of the problems of cleft palate repair. adenoids should be left whenever possible. although many clinics claim 80% normal speech after cleft palate repair, 50% is probably a good indication of a correct approach by a competent team. the post operative appearance of the palate may bear no relation to the quality of the speech produced. obturators may be uncomfortable and even harmful if their fitting and maintenance v are not submitted to frequent checks. obturators of a special kind, with thick centre sections may, in some cases, help to displace the tongue in a favourable manner. sub-mucous clefts are not rare and, although they present the appearance of a closed palate, the muscles are not attached in the mid-line and pull away between the layers of oral and nasal mucous membrane during speech. this failure of normal action is partlyj responsible for the "cleft palate" speech produced by these patients. median cleft lip is rarely seen. two abnormalities associated with varying degrees of post-alveolar clefts are:— (i) the pierre robin syndrome — characterised by a receding lower jaw and serious episodes of chocking when feeding the new born child is attempted, (ii) acrocephalo-syndactyly is a condition of a tall skull, flattened from before backwards in a child with fused fingers and toes and a posterior cleft palate. these conditions are, of course, congenital. the more intelligent the patient, the more subtle and hopeful can be the teaching. opsomming dit word vandag algemeen aangeneem dat een persoon alleen nie die beste behandeling moontlik aan pasiente met gesplete lip en verhemelte kan gee nie. die span sal uit drie groepe bestaan:— (1) plastiese sjirurge en verpleegsters. (2) orthodontiste. (3) spraakterapeute. omtrent 20% van alle gesplete lip en verhemelte pasiente het 'n familiegeskiedenis van hierdie toestand. van die orige 80%, kry ons 'n groot aantal pasiente in families waar ondervoeding voorkom. die rol wat virus en ander infeksies speel is onseker. totdat die effek van steroides meer bekend is, behoort dit baie versigtig en onder deeglike toesig alleenlik, aan swanger vrouens toegedien te word — veral gedurende die eerste drie maande van swangerskap. hierdie gebrek kom gemiddeld een uit 'n duisend gevalle voor. die aanvangsbehandeling kan al in die kraaminrigting plaasvind. die orthodontis begin om die alveolarelyn te vorm met 'n klein spalkie, wat met rekbande aan 'n weefselagtige mus vas is. die daaropvolgende operasies is dan gewoonlik makliker en meer suksesvol. om meer duidelikheid te verkry in verband met klassifikasie voor die operasie, mag dit beter wees om 'n kort standaard beskrywing, eerder as die gewone handboekbenaminge te gebruik. operasies vir haaslip en gesplete verhemelte is selde, indien ooit, noodgevallepasiente, met selfs die ergste graad van splete, kan andersinds nog 'n gesonde volwassenheid bereik, met weinig of geen behandeling nie — hoewel dit nooit vandag december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) behoort te gebeur nie. daar is nie 'n spesifieke ouderdom, wanneer dit die beste is om die herstelwerk te doen nie — gesaghebbendes verskil nog oor die onderwerp. die algemene idee is dat die beste resultate gekry word wanneer herstelwerk aan die lip na die ouderdom van drie maande gedoen word en aan die verhemelte, voor twee jaar. daar is gegewens wat bewys dat die groeiproses, van die middelste derde van die gesig, minder gesteur word, as die operasie op die verhemelte nie baie groot is nie, en/of as dit uitgestel word tot die ouderdom van omtrent vier jaar. die doel van lipherstellingswerk is om 'n fyn wit litteken te kry, met die vloer van die neusvleuels op dieselfde horisontale vlak en die basis van die neusvleuels behoort ewe ver van die middellyn aan elke kant van die kollumella af te wees. die rooi lyn moet ook egalig wees van links na regs, sonder enige „trappies" en die slymvlies moet dieselfde diepte he aan elke kant: die kante van die spleet behoort ewe lank te wees. die pasient bly gewoonlik vir tien dae in die hospitaal. die hoofdoel van die herstelling van 'n gesplete verhemelte is om normale spraak te verseker. om dit te bereik behoort die operasie 'n lang, beweeglike sagteverhemelte met goeie spierverbindings -te lewer. die sagte verhemelte is belangrik vir die afsluiting tussen de neus-keelholte en mond-keelholte gedurende spraak. wanneer alleenlik aan die sagteverhemelte gewerk word, is bevind dat die v-y of wardill operasie meer bevredigend is as die langenbeck operasie. die wardill „vier-flap" operasie, met behulp van die septum se slymvlies, kan ook gebruik word, by groter splete, maar moet die alveolare rif nog onbeskadig wees. een van die beste metodes om 'n lang verhemelte, met 'n minimale gevaar van 'n fistel, te verseker, is die cuthbert „drie-flap" operasie. die twee agterste lappe is nie ewe lank nie en die herstelling van die hardeverhemelte sluit die derde lap, wat voor vas is, in. die eerste stadium van die herstelling van totale splete, uniof bilateraal, sluit 'n grootskaalse gebruik van die slymvlies, aan beide kante van die septum, in. as dit suksesvol is, sal die operasie in die tweede stadium, na 'n paar maande, net die sluiting van die sagteverhemelte beteken. twee faktore beinvloed gewoonlik die sukses van die sjirurgie: tydsberekening en tegniekop voorwaarde dat die pasient reg opgesom en geskik vir operasie is. tydsberekening en tegniek is belangrik by al drie die bogenoemde groepe. oor die algemeen het minder kinders spraakterapie nodig as die operasies vroeg en voldoende uitgevoer word. wanneer die operasie gelaat word totdat die kind al 'n hele aantal woorde verkeerd uitspreek, sal so 'n kind omtrent altyd sukkel om normale spraak aan te leer. wanneer dit duidelik is dat ouers die grondbeginsels van die behandeling verstaan moet dit aan hulle verduidelik word. daar sonder moeilikheid van 'n paar weke tot drie maande gewag word voordat met spraakterapie begin word, as die swelsels en littekens in die verhemelte lank neem om te genees. gedurende hierdie tyd is dit soms gewens om die kind toe te laat om te probeer praat, sonder dat hy behandeling ontvang — slegs om hom die geleentheid te gun om onder die normale omstandighede, van nabootsing, te leer praat. spraakterapie is gewoonlik nie skadelik in enige van die stadiums nie. die enige moeilikheid is intensiewe behandeling om die tong te gebruik om spraakklanke te vorm en beheer met 'n fistel voor, terwyl normale spraak met die res van die tong en verhemelte gevorm word. dit is onwaarskynlik dat enige gevaar bestaan as daar met spraakterapie begin word langer as twee weke na die operasie. in die geval van 'n gesplete verhemelte, behoort mangels alleenlik deur 'n dokter, met kennis van die probleme wat spruit uit die herstelling van 'n gesplete verhemelte verwyder te word. indien moontlik, behoort die adenoide nie verwyder te word nie. alhoewel baie klinieke daarop roem dat hulle 80% normale spraak kry na operasies aan 'n gesplete verhemelte, is 50% 'n goeie 1 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) aanduiding van die regte benadering deur 'n bekwame span. die voorkoms van die verhemelte na die operasie is geen aanduiding van die kwaliteit van de spraak wat gevorm sal word nie. obturators kan ongemaklik en selfs gevaarlik wees as hulle nie pas nie, en as die versorging nie gedurig gekontroleer word nie. 'n sekere soort obturator, met 'n dik middeldeel, mag in sommige gevalle help met die plasing van die tong. splete, wat verberg word deur 'n normale slymvlies in die mond en neus, is nie 'n rare verskynsel nie en omdat die spiere, op die middellyn, nie verbind is nie, trek hulle uit mekaar as die persoon praat. hierdie gebrek aan normale aksie veroorsaak die tipe spraak wat by 'n persoon met 'n gesplete verhemelte gevind word. 'n sentrale spleet kom selde voor. twee abnormaliteite word geassosieer met verskillende grade van splete wat agter die alveolare rif voorkom:— (i) die pierre-robin sindroom —' herkenbaar aan 'n kort kakebeen en ernstige aanvalle van verstikking wanneer die pasgebore baba gevoed word. (ii) akrokefalo-sidaktilie is 'n toestand van 'n kort kakebeen en 'n lang kopbeen wat plat van voor af agtertoe loop, by 'n kind met vingers en tone wat saamgesmelt is en 'n gesplete verhemelte agter in die mond. hierdie toestande is natuurlik aangebore. hoe intelligenter die pasient is, hoe meer subtiel en veelbelowend is die onderrig. acknowledgements the author wishes to thank mr. j. b. cuthbert, head of the plastic surgery unit of the johannesburg group of hospitals, for many stimulating discussions and demonstrations; also professor dreyer and dr. g. gavron of the dental and oral hospital for information from their departments. in addition thanks are due to members of the staff of the speech therapy department of the johannesburg general hospital for their co-operation in assessing and handling many patients. december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) page six journal of the south african logopedic society a method of achieving carryover of relaxation into life situations audrey shavell b.a. log. muscular relaxation as a therapeutic agent in the treatment of speech disorders has been widely used by speech therapists. the various methods of teaching relaxation are well known to speech therapists and do not warrant repetition of fundamental principles. this article is concerned with a method of obtaining carryover of the relaxed feeling and attitude into social situations. one often finds in general logopedic practice a patient who successfully masters tne tecnnique of general bodily, relaxation and differential relaxation in the clinic situation, but who finds it very difficult to obtain a carryover of relaxation into everyday living. a method of 'easing' the difficulties both children and adults experience in this direction which the writer found helpful will now be described. when the patient has reached the stage where he can relax voluntarily in the clinic situation i introduce the technique of psycho-drama i.e. we act out common everyday life situations in both a tense and a relaxed manner. we then analyse whether the tension is of any use to the speaker or serves any purpose in the speech. there is usually some initial resistance in the older child and adult to acting things 'out' but . this is soon overcome and the patient is then ready to tackle extra-clinic assignments. the first extra-clinic assignments are usually reallife situations that have previously been acted out at the clinic e.g. the dinner table, talking on the telephone, being kept waiting for an appointment at the doctor or dentist etc. etc. reports on the success of the assignments are written and then the situation is enacted at the clinic in both a tense and an easy manner. older children and adults then make a detailed assessment of the overt reactions of others to the forced tense speech or the smooth easy speech. most of the assignments are concerned with assisting the carryover of relaxation into life situations. this method is particularly successful in groups and has been used by the writer for all types of hypertense speech defectives. two cases have been chosen to illustrate the use of this method with patients of a widely differing age range and type of disorder. case a. a 25 year old adult male secondary stutterer was given a thorough and well planned mental hygiene programme to help him learn to 'know himself' and to eliminate 'mal-attitudes' towards himself and his speech. after four months of treatment a re-assessment of the patient's attitude towards his defect showed a decided increase in objectivity. the case was then taught relaxation using a combination of the fink and jacobson methods. complete voluntary relaxation was learned in approximately seven weeks, but the patient achieved little 'natural carryover' of relaxation into extra-clinic situations. the difficulties of obtaining carryover were aggravated by the fact that the case was extremely quick tempered and frequently flew into a rage before the thought of relaxation entered his head. (he appeared to have a very low frustration tolerance level.) the previously described method of achieving carryover of relaxation was used exclusively for four weeks with this case, by which time carryover into social situations was almost complete and a marked change in both personality and speech of the subject was noticeable. severity of the.stutter and duration of the blocks had decreased. after four months of therapy devoted to the elimination .of the actual stuttering symptoms, the case was successfully dismissed rehabilitated. he has been rechecked four times at six monthly intervals since his dismissal from the clinic and appears to have been completely rehabilitated. case b. a highly intelligent seven-year old severe tensionathetoid was actually taught relaxation through the previously described method of psychodrama. games like pretending to live in 'slow easy land' where we met 'sleepy joe', 'floppy flo', 'raggedy ann' and plop the rag doll or living in the 'land of the melting snowman' were used to introduce the idea of relaxation to a child to whom relaxation was so foreign. after the child had mastered the technique of relaxation, carryover of relaxation was worked on through acting out life situations in an 'easy relaxed' way. the therapist then accompanied the child into life situations (in the school-situation and through gentle reminders or 'secret looks' helped the child release excess tension in these situations. this was considered to be part of a secret game between the therapist and the child.) when the child had obtained a fair degree of carryover of relaxation in the school situation the mother was let into the 'secret' of 'slow easy land' and the 'land of the melting snowman' and taught to be like the inhabitants of these lands, to help the child obtain carryover of relaxation into the home situation. the writer does not believe that relaxation training is a 'panacea' to speech therapists, but that specific methods of ensuring, carryover of relaxation into social situations must be used for those patients who are taught general bodily relaxation. we so often find a case who relaxes well in the clinic situation but is quite unable to utilise his ability to relax in social situations, where he needs it most. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y sub-mucous cleft palate case histories by d. m. whiting, b.a.. log. (rand). l.c.s.t. the occurrence of sub-mucous cleft palate is rare, but even so, it is vitally important that the speech therapist be aware of the condition as a possible cause of nasal speech. she should be able to recognise the symptoms which lead to its diagnosis and refer all suspected cases to a surgeon who deals with cleft lip and palate for consultation before undertaking treatment. it has been my experience that sub-mucous cleft palate is a condition unknown and unrecognised by many in general medical, surgical and dental practice, whereas it is quickly recognised by surgeons concerned with the treatment of cleft lip and palate. the following case histories are presented to illustrate this observation, and to stress the importance of accurate diagnosis by the speech therapist in the handling of these cases. case a. a european girl, 14 years of age. she was referred for speech therapy three years previously with nasal speech. she received treatment at school for one year and at clinic for one year. further treatment was recommended but transport difficulties made this impossible. improvement was negligible. during the second year of treatment the possibility of a sub-mucous cleft palate was suggested, but not verified. on examination: hard palate—there was no bony union for the posterior two-thirds of the hard palate. this v-shap'ed notch of the posterior border was easily visible and palpation was unnecessary. the oral mucosa was intact. soft palate—there was a medial mucous line about ; 1/1 oth of an inch wide running from the base of the v of the hard palate the full length of the soft palate, which widened on phonation. this mucous line glowed red on illumination of the nasopharynx, confirming the absence of muscle union. the palate appeared short and the uvula was bifid. velopharyngeal closure was negative on tests and in speech. articulation was correct but weak. there was consistent nasal tone. this diagnosis of sub-mucous cleft palate was confirmed by the plastic surgery unit, and surgery was undertaken. further speech therapy should have followed immediately, but the girl had lost all interest and incentive and failed to attend in spite of arrangements being made to facilitate transport. had she been given the opportunity of surgical repair 2—3 years earlier, instead of the constant failure to improve with speech therapy, the results would probably have been more encouraging. cast b. an indian girl aged 9 years. her home language was ghurgurati and she attended an english medium school. she was referred from the dental hospital for examination and treatment on account of her very limited language ability. she presented a complicated picture. her birth and early history and subsequent neurological examinations revealed the probability of her being a brain injured child. she was very small for her age and there was a history of earlier tubercular infection. at birth the nose appeared flattened and was described by the father as being "almost absent." feeding proved very difficult as the child could not suck, and there was regurgitation through the nose. there was no family history of cleft palate or speech defects. an examination at another speech clinic two years earlier, failed to reveal any palate abnormality. on examination: nose—bridge appeared flattened and nasal airways narrow. teeth—malocclusion and abnormal structure of upper and lower incisors, (receiving treatment at time of examination). hard palate—there was a high narrow arch with a marked v shaped notch in the centre of the posterior border. soft palate—this appeared short and fairly mobile, there was a bifid uvula. velopharyngeal closure was negative both on tests and in speech. there was an overall nasal tone and nasal escape. articulation was grossly defective. it seemed to me that this was a case of submucous cleft palate and/or congenital short palate, in addition to a severe language retardation probably due to brain damage. however, controversial opinions were expressed regarding her palatal condition by others concerned with her treatment. she was then referred to a plastic surgeon for r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j 6 u f t n a l _ o f f l i £ s o u t h a f r i c a n l 0 0 0 p e d i c s o c i l g t v a p r i l α further opinion. he reported that this was certainly a case of sub-mucous cleft palate and that the soft palate was definitely short. on account of the child's generally poor physical condition, he advised that the case should be reconsidered for the possibility of surgical repair three years later. this examination should take place in 1957. it was decided to treat the language disturbance in the meantime, in spite of the complication of grossly defective articulation and language medium. case c. a coloured boy aged 11 years. this child was referred for treatment by an ear, nose and throat department, where he was receiving treatment for a discharging right ear. he was referred as a repaired cleft lip and palate. this was only partly true, his lip had been repaired at 2 years of age, but there had been no further operations of any kind. his young stepmother could supply no information concerning his early development and possible feeding difficulties. a sister, six years younger, had a repaired double cleft lip and palate. no other family incidence of cleft palate or speech defect was reported. the boy was very selfconscious of his grossly defective speech and fought back when teased. when examined he was found to be a friendly co-operative child and very anxious to be helped. on examination: lip—there was a repaired right cleft lip, the lip was mobile. teeth—right upper premolar and incisors were crooked. hard palate—there was a high narrow arch particularly anteriorly, and a marked v shaped notch medially at the posterior border. soft palate—there was a central transparent mucous line which widened on phonation, and appeared as a red line when the nasopharynx was illuminated, the palate appeared short with limited mobility and there was a bifid uvula. velopharyngeal closure was negative on tests and in speech. articulation was grossly defective with frequent use of the glottal stop and speech was frequently unintelligible. there was an overall nasal tone. the provisional diagnosis was sub-mucous cleft palate associated with a right cleft lip this was later confirmed by the plastic surgery unit, where he was put on the list for surgical repair. speech therapy was to be postponed until after the operation. case d. a european boy of 6 years of age. he and his sister were in the care of child welfare. his mother was deceased and no early history was available. his sister had a repaired cleft of the soft palate and normal speech with the exception of sigmatism. the boy's was a case complicated by multiple congenital abnormalities, spina bifida oculta, hypertonia and dextrocardia with a cystolic murmur. a few months before my examination the child had had encephalitis, and about six months after examination poliomyelitis (non-paralytic). he was very small for his age and had not yet been to school. he had been examined a few months earlier with a view to special treatment but no mention was made of a palatal abnormality. on examination: teeth—teeth were in a very poor condition and the upper incisors had been extracted. hard palate—there was no medial notch of the posterior border, but the shape of the posterior border resembled a wide v based anteriorly and spreading laterally to the regions of the hamular process. (i have only once before seen this in a case of congenially short palate). the soft palate appeared short with limited mobility, and on illumination of the nasopharynx a red mucous line appeared down the full length of the soft palate. the uvula was bifid. the pharynx was wide laterally. velopharyngeal closure was negative. articulation—there were some articulatory errors, but speech was intelligible. there was an overall nasality. the provisional diagnosis of sub-mucous cleft of the soft palate was later confirmed by the plastic surgery unit. there was no question of operation for the time being, in this case, on account of his general physical condition and multiple abnormalities. it was decided he should receive a short period of speech therapy to ascertain what improvement could be expected in spite of the condition. i should like to thank dr. medalie for his permission to investigate case d, and the plastic surgery unit, johannesburg hospital group for their help with all four cases. i should also like to thank professor p. de v. pienaar, director of the university speech, voice and hearing clinic, and the medical superintendent of the johannesburg general hospital, for permission to report on these cases. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 t h e editor teacher to make contact with the child, gain his confidence and inculcate a desire and love of 'learning' by helping the child to achieve success. the importance of the teacher understanding 'how a child learns' does not apply only to those who handle the slow learner, brain-damaged or retarded children. there are many children in normal schools who would be saved feelings of failure, frustration, fear and from becoming problem children if helped to achieve success in their first years at school. teaching, with a challenge, becomes really worth while! opsomming daar is indiwiduele verskille in die leerproses, selfs onder die sogenaamde normale kinders. die verskille word egter duideliker as ons te doen het met die afwykende kind. die verstandelik vertraagde kind sal stadiger vorder en nie so ver vorder as die normale kind nie. die breinbeseerde sal weer op sekere gebiede vinniger en op ander gebiede stadiger vorder a.g.v. die letsel. hulle mag probleme ten opsigte van die integrasieproses en interpretasie bied. om alle bykomende afwykings bv. gehoorverlies, uit te sluit, is spanwerk nodig. 'n volledige diagnose moet van elke pasient gemaak word. in die behandeling van 'n kind met persepsieprobleme kan gebruik gemaak word van taal en spraak om persepsie te verbeter bv. die verbalisasie van handbewegings in die uitvoering van 'n psigomotoriese aksie. die belang van basiese vaardighede wat die kind gereed maak vir die leerproses in die skool kan nie genoeg beklemtoon word nie. ook moet die kind in staat wees om te herken, analiseer, sintetiseer, herroep en instruksies te volg (visueel en oiiditief). die taak van die onderwyser wat werk met die breinbeseerde kind, is 'n uitdaging wat die moeite werd is. reference kephart, newell c. (i960): the slow learner in the classroom. columbus, o h i o : charles e. merrill books, inc. discussion of mrs. ε. m. harrison's paper t h e e d i t o r / ten years ago mrs. harrison, using a technique which she has termed directional training, experienced success when attempting to teach braininjured children to learn to write. it is interesting to examine this technique in the light of a recent postulate of the eminent russian psychologist and neuro-psychiatrist, professor luria. luria has long been concerned with the application of experimental method to the problems of behaviour, and rank's as an experimental psychologist of high repute. his interest in language is demonstrated by the importance which journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 12, no. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) discussion on mrs. ε. m. harrison's paper 19 he attaches to the study of child development. in his opinion the study of language and its development is the key to our understanding of vital problems in human intellectual growth. in his book the role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behaviour, luria has shown that the perception of a child can be controlled and modified through speech. this idea is contrary to the conventional way of thinking about perception as being merely a reaction which takes place through a sense organ. in order to substantiate this point of view, luria quotes certain experiments. experiment i. children aged 12-30 months were given small red and green boxes, the green empty and the red containing sweets. it proved very difficult for the children to select the correct boxes (i.e. those containing sweets) in a choice situation. however, when speech entered the experiment by the experimenter naming the colours of the two boxes and then asking the children to select the correct one, the significant cue stood out boldly and the children were able to make a correct selection. this experiment clearly demonstrates how speech can substantially modify a child's perception and by so doing permit "the working out of a system of stable differentiated associations." luria has further demonstrated this regulatory role which speech exerts on behaviour in his experimental work with brain-injured patients. of particular interest are his experiments with patients suffering from parkinsonism, where the motor system is directly affected but where, as luria states, the "verbal system" is considerably intact. experiment 2. a subject suffering from parkinsonism was asked to answer the following questions with his fingers: i. how many wheels are there on a car ? ii. how many brothers have you ? iii. how many points on the red star ? whereas formerly the patient had lost movement in his hand, when answering the foregoing questions movement was regained. according to luria what had occurred was that although the hands had lost their primary function, when used for the purpose of answering questions, they entered a complex verbal functional system where the defective movement was compensated for. in fact, by adding a new system of intact verbal afferentation to the defective movement, it is possible to switch the process to a new level and attain, as luria postulated, "a form of self-regulation" of a process which was previously inaccessible. returning to mrs. harrison's work, it seems apparent that her technique of directional training, where the children she taught verbalized while carrying out movement and interpreted verbally movements made by other children, can be closely linked to luria's ideas. she, too, was in fact .linking a relatively intact verbal system to a system which was defective, and by so doing enabled her pupils to compensate for their disabilities. it is indeed commendable that ten years ago mrs. harrison should have devised this technique, the theory behind which has only become accessible to us in a work published for the first time in 1961. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 12, nr. 1 .· september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 the editor opsomming die tegnieke wat mev. ε. m. harrison gebruik om sekere breinbeseerde kinders wat nie fisies gestrem is nie, te leer skryf, is in verband gebring met die werk en bevindings van die beroemde russiese sielkundige en neuropsigiater, prof. a. r. luria. luria stel dit dat spraak 'n regulerende invloed uitoefen op 'n kind se gedrag en 'n spesifieke invloed het op die ontwikkeling van sy verstandsprosesse. . deur die gebruik van die rigtingwysende opleiding het mev. harrison bewys dat breinbeseerde kinders, sonder fisiese stremming, maar wat nie deur die gebruiklike metodes kon leer skryf nie, tog die vaardigheid kon aanleer sodra as spraak met die spesifieke aksie van skryf verbind word. op dieselfde wyse het luria bewys dat sekere gevalle van parkinsonism? in staat gestel word om hulle hande suksesvol te gebruik wanneer die spesifieke beweging verbind word met die verbale meganismes. blykbaar word, deur die byvoeging van 'n nuwe stelsel van onbelemmerde verbale afferentasie by die afwykende beweging, 'n verskuiwing van die proses na η nuwe en suksesvolle peil bewerkstellig. hierdie peil kan nie deur die konvensionele opleidingsmetodes bereik word nie. references luria, a. r. (1961): the role of speech in the regulation of normal and abnormal behaviour. london: pergamon press. , , „ , . . . , luria, a. r. and yudowich, f. ia. (1959): speech and the development of mental processes in the child. london: staples press. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 12, no. 1: september 1965 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) article information authors: hanlie degenaar1 alta kritzinger2 affiliations: 1institute of psychology and wellbeing, north-west university, potchefstroom campus, south africa 2department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa correspondence to: hanlie degenaar email: hanlie.degenaar@nwu.ac.za postal address: po box 6297, flamwood, klerksdorp 2572, south africa dates: received: 17 dec. 2014 accepted: 27 apr. 2015 published: 02 dec. 2015 how to cite this article: degenaar, h., & kritzinger, a. (2015). suck, swallow and breathing coordination in infants with infantile colic. south african journal of communication disorders, 62(1), art. #115, 10 pages. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v62i1.115 copyright notice: © 2015. the authors. licensee: aosis openjournals. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. suck, swallow and breathing coordination in infants with infantile colic in this original research... open access • abstract • introduction • aims • method    • participants    • material and data collection    • data analysis    • reliability and validity • results • discussion • conclusion • acknowledgements    • competing interests    • authors’ contributions • references • appendix a    • assessment protocol for suck, swallow and breathing coordination (ssbc) abstract top ↑ background: there appears to be a perception amongst parents and in popular literature that infantile colic is caused by feeding difficulties. limited support for this perception is found in scientific literature. whilst there is scientific evidence that suck, swallow and breathing are key components of successful feeding, these components and the coordination thereof in infants with colic have not been extensively researched. objective: the objective of the study was to explore the suck, swallow and breathing coordination in infants with infantile colic and compare it with infants without the condition. method: an assessment protocol for suck, swallow and breathing coordination was compiled from literature. this protocol was performed on a research group of 50 infants, independently diagnosed with infantile colic, and a control group of 28 infants without the condition. all participants were from two rural towns in the north–west province, south africa, selected with a snowball selection method and strict selection criteria. the study followed a static comparison group design. results: a significant difference in the key components of feeding and the presence of colic in participants of four age categories were found. the correlation between postural control and the presence of infantile colic were sustained in participants from 2–19 weeks old. conclusion: suck, swallow and breathing were found to be significantly associated with infantile colic. the findings should be investigated further. it appears that speech-language therapists may play an expanding role in infantile colic. introduction top ↑ infantile colic is a condition that commonly occurs in 10%–40% of typical, healthy and growing infants whether they are breastfeeding or bottle feeding (deshpande, 2003; kheir, 2012; søndergaard, skajaa & henriksen, 2000) but lasts only until the age of four months (cohen-silver & ratnapalan, 2009; kheir, 2012; savino, 2007). the description of infantile colic mostly used in literature is still based on the definition of wessel, cobb, jackson, harris and detwiler (1954). the condition is described as sudden onset periods of high-pitched crying without an explainable cause (kheir, 2012), exceeding three hours per day in duration (deshpande, 2003; gudmundsson, 2010) and lasting for more than three days within a period of three weeks (lucassen et al., 2001; savino, 2007). several factors have already been identified that may increase the risk of infantile colic. these include gastro-oesophageal reflux (heine, 2006), increased levels of gastrointestinal hormones (savino et al., 2006), flora (savino et al., 2010), esophagitis (berezin, glassman, bostwick & halata, 1995), low birth weight (søndergaard et al., 2000), maternal smoking (reijneveld, brugman & hirasing, 2000), lactose intolerance (kanabar, randhawa & clayton, 2001) and feeding difficulties (gudmundsson, 2010; miller-loncar, bigsby, high, wallach & lester, 2004). the etiology appears to be unknown (kheir, 2012; lucassen et al., 2001) and no standard treatment protocol for infantile colic has been indicated (hall, chesters & robinson, 2012). despite limited clinical evidence that feeding problems occur in infants with infantile colic (miller-loncar et al., 2004) the perception amongst the general public and the popular literature are that difficulties with sucking and swallowing causes colic (bailey, d’ auria & haushalter, 2012). advertisements and articles in baby magazines reinforce the perception that colic can be alleviated by certain bottles, teats and a change in handling the infant (catherine, ko & barr, 2008). this difference in the common perception of infantile colic and clinical evidence has not been investigated. successful feeding is determined by three factors, namely the infant’s oral-motor feeding movements, the sucking, swallowing and breathing coordination (ssbc) and the interaction during feeding with the caregiver (hall, 2001; morris & klein, 2001; swigert, 2009). it is generally accepted that infants with infantile colic have normal sucking and swallowing skills, adequate growth and adequate nutrition (deshpande, 2003; lucassen, 2010). miller-loncar et al. (2004) however found that infants with infantile colic had less rhythmic sucking, organised feeding behaviour and interactive responses during feeding than a control group. there is also evidence that the condition negatively impacts on infant-caregiver interaction (brown, thoyre, pridham & schubert, 2009; miller-loncar et al., 2004; rossetti, 2001) and adds to parental frustration (deshpande, 2003; hall et al., 2012), postnatal depression (vik et al., 2009), family stress (beebe, casey & pinto-martin, 1993) and family conflict (raiha, lehtonen, korhonen & korvenranta, 1997). based on these results there may be a relationship between infantile colic, ssbc and caregiver interaction. the feeding process, in particular ssbc, has not yet been investigated in infants with infantile colic. successful feeding in an infant younger than four months depends on a well-developed ssbc pattern (arvedson & brodsky, 2002; wolf & glass, 1992). at four months most neonatal reflexes, the moro, rooting, sucking, tonic neck reflex and palmar grasp, disappear (alexander, boehme & cupps, 1993; morris & klein, 2001) and successful feeding is less dependent on ssbc. the integration of reflexes into typical movement patterns may explain why infantile colic eases or disappears at age four months (savino & tarasco, 2010) and also strengthens the idea that ssbc may play a role in the condition. ssbc is a fundamental sensory motor pattern which organises the infant’s neuro-motor behaviour (oetter, richter & frick, 1995) and is present since birth in typical full-term infants (swigert, 2009). ssbc is considered as the first development pattern that involves successive, timed and sequenced movement of different structures (barlow, 2009) with a significant influence on the infant’s postural control, psychosocial development and emotional state (brown et al., 2009; oetter et al., 1995). that is because ssbc involves various bony structures, muscles, cervical and cranial nerves (barlow, 2009; seikel, douglas & drumright, 2010) and is also linked to the limbic system, reticular formation and autonomic nervous system (oetter et al., 1995; wolf & glass, 1992). a disturbance in ssbc could by implication disturb the infant’s sleep patterns, alertness, attention and sensory threshold (blanche, botticelli & hallway 1995; hemmi, wolke & schneider, 2011; oetter et al., 1995). ssbc is a complex, synchronised movement pattern for feeding in infants and involves three functional components synchronised by the hyoid complex. figure 1 displays the relationship between the three components of ssbc. figure 1: the relationship between the different components of ssak. as indicated in figure 1 the hyoid bone and the muscles attached to the structure are central to ssbc. the suprahyoid and infrahyoid muscles stabilise the hyoid bone which should be aligned with other bony structures involved in ssbc to achieve effective sequential movements (morris & klein, 2001; perkins & kent, 1986; wolf & glass, 1992). the hyoid bone provides coordination of muscle movement around the bony structures involved in sucking, swallowing and breathing (oetter et al., 1995, perkins & kent, 1986; seikel et al., 2010). any disturbance of the hyoid complex will disturb ssbc and a slight disturbance in ssbc may lead to a slight disturbance in the infant’s feeding process. the conclusion is that a subtle disturbance in the balance between the components of ssbc may cause a number of feeding difficulties in young infants. clinical observable factors could assist to identify and assess a ssbc disturbance. the diagnosis of infantile colic is currently characterised by parental perception of the infant’s behaviour and the elimination of other medical conditions (deshpande, 2003; kanabar, 2008; savino & tarasco, 2010) without reference to the feeding process. parents base their perception of colic on the acoustic characteristic of the infant’s cry and behaviour of fisting, flatulence and pulling legs towards the abdomen (deshpande, 2003; lester, boukydis, garcia-coll, hole & peucker, 1992; savino, 2007; st james-roberts, conroy & wilsher, 1996). parental descriptions of colic vary as perceptions are determined by socio-economic status, education, religion, previous experience of an infant with infantile colic, environmental factors, personality, parental age, marital status and the presence of a support system (rossetti, 2001). differences in descriptions are therefore not objective and reliable for assessment of the condition. observation of factors that may disturb ssbc may contribute to objectivity in the assessment of infants with infantile colic. postural control, feeding position, sucking rhythm and cranio-cervical position are four observable factors that determine the effective functioning of the hyoid complex and the ultimately ssbc (barlow, 2009; oetter et al., 1995; wolf & glass, 1992). the observable factors that influence ssbc are depicted in figure 2. figure 2: observable factors involved in disturbance of sucking, swallowing and breathing coordination. according to figure 2, postural control is the ability to align bony structures and maintain alignment during an activity (cupps, 1997) and is therefore essential for feeding (arvedson & lefton-greif, 1996; hall, 2001; morris & klein, 2001; rogers, 1996). poor alignment leads to less efficient feeding, increased energy expenditure, limited endurance and prolonged duration of feeding (hall, 2001; morris & klein, 2001; wolf & glass, 1992). feeding position is important since any external force on the hyoid complex or bony structures involved in breathing may disturb ssbc (morris & klein, 2001; oetter et al., 1995; perkins & kent, 1986). several authors have stressed the importance of the feeding position for infants (arvedson & lefton-greif, 1996; finnie, 1992; hall, 2001; harris, 1986; morris & klein, 2001; rogers, 1996; swigert, 2009; wolf & glass, 1992). sucking rhythm is determined by the overlapping nature of ssbc innervations from the cranial nerves (trigeminal, facial, glosso-pharyngeal, vagal, accessorius, hypoglossus), cervical nerves 1–7 and thoracic nerves 1–12 (seikel et al., 2010; wolf & glass, 1992). the overlapping function ensures the synergetic, rhythmic and synchronous flow between sucking, swallowing and breathing in infants (barlow, 2009; oetter et al., 1995; wolf & glass, 1992). any disturbance in the innervations of one of the components of ssbc may cause arhythmic sucking, swallowing or breathing, which will be an observable indication of a disturbance in ssbc. the cranio-cervical position is the alignment of the head and neck with slight neck flexion that optimally opens both the oesophagus and trachea (morris & klein, 2001; wolf & glass, 1992). if a neutral cranio-cervical position is not maintained, the mobility of the hyoid bone is affected (wolf & glass, 1992), thereby causing a disturbance in ssbc. it is clear that postural control, postural alignment (including cranio-cervical position and feeding position) and sucking rhythm should be included in a clinical assessment. infantile colic is associated with an increase risk for psychosocial conditions such as postnatal depression in the mother (vik et al., 2009), poor mother-infant interaction (brown et al., 2009), sleep disturbances and tantrums in the infant (hemmi et al., 2011), infant difficulties with emotional regulation (gomez, baird & jung, 2004), family strain and poor family relationships (canivet, jakobsson & hagander, 2000; räihä, lehtonen, huhtala, saleva & korvenranta, 2002). the presence of infantile colic is also associated with an increased risk for infant neglect, abuse, being shaken and death (barr, trent & cross, 2006). these factors suggest a continuum of risks (rossetti, 2001) in the infant and the family, which may influence early communication development and psychological well-being. difficulties that impact negatively on the development of swallowing and feeding skills often contribute to educational difficulties later in life (mckirdy, sheppard, osborne & payne, 2008), justifying the inclusion of infants with infantile colic in early communication intervention programmes. the presence of ssbc difficulties and the clinical assessment thereof may guide intervention and future research into the role of speech-language therapists in infants with infantile colic. aims top ↑ to explore the feeding in infants with infantile colic, the research had two aims. the first aim was to compile a clinical assessment protocol for ssbc. the second aim was to clinically assess and describe ssbc in a group of infants with colic (research group) and compare the findings with a group of infants without the condition (control group). method top ↑ ethical clearance was obtained from the research ethics committee of the faculty of humanities at the university of pretoria. all participants gave informed consent. for the first aim a literature study was conducted. for the second aim a comparative two-group research design was used to clinically observe ssbc in a group of infant participants who were independently diagnosed with the condition, in contrast with a control group without the condition. a non-randomised sample was selected of infants referred by local clinics or medical practitioners to a speech-language therapy practice in two rural towns in the north-west province of south africa. correlation coefficients were calculated to determine whether relationships exist between the ssbc in a group of infants with colic and those without the condition. participants a research group of 50 infant participants with colic and 28 control participants without colic were selected using a snowball selection procedure according to four age categories. the participants with infantile colic were independently diagnosed by their medical practitioners according to the wessel et al.’ s (1954) definition of the condition. the selection criteria were as follows: the infants had to be between 1 and 17 weeks old and born at 37 weeks gestation or later, as literature indicates the condition is present in infants 0–4 months old (savino & tarasco, 2010). the prenatal history was required to determine the presence of risk factors. no risk factors such as low birth weight, poor weight gain, growth retardation, prematurity, maternal smoking, congenital anomaly or any neonatal medical conditions (allergy, reflux, gastrointestinal difficulties and esophagitis) should have been present. participants should not have been using any medication, as this could influence behaviour and may have decreased the reliability of observations. the infants should have been cared for by the parents during the day, so that parental reporting on infantile colic would be reliable. the infants could be breastfed or bottle fed as literature indicates that infantile colic occurs in both breastfed and bottle fed infants (deshpande, 2003). the participants came from different socio-economic groups. some participants only had access to their community clinic where fees were minimal whilst other had access to private medical services. table 1a–d displays the characteristics of the participants. table 1a: description of participants according age category 1: 2-4 weeks (n = 26) table 1b: description of participants according age category 2: 5-8 weeks (n = 24) table 1c: description of participants according age category 3: 9-12 weeks (n = 14) table 1d: description of participants according age category 4: 13-19 weeks (n = 14) according to table 1 the participants in the two groups were fairly similar regarding gender and birth weight, but differed greatly regarding duration and frequency of feeds. fewer participants in the research group than in the control group were breastfed. material and data collection an assessment protocol was compiled from feeding assessment forms in literature (arvedson & brodsky, 2002; swigert, 2009; wolf & glass, 1992). descriptions of postural control in infants were added (alexander et al., 1993; bly, 1995). the content of the assessment protocol is described in table 2. the final assessment protocol is included in appendix 1. table 2: content of the assessment protocol for sucking, swallowing and breathing coordination. all participants were observed in prone, supine, supported standing and supported sitting for the appropriate postural control and alignment (alexander et al., 1993; bly, 1995; hall, 2001; swigert, 2009) followed by eliciting nutritive sucking. feeding by the mother was observed. the researchers have combined experience in the field of paediatric dysphagia and received training in neurodevelopmental assessment and therapy for infants, as well as neurodevelopmental care for preterm infants. a nominal value was given to absence or presence of items on the assessment protocol. data analysis participants were divided in age categories of 2–4, 5–8, 9–12 and 13–19 weeks old. since the number of participants in the different age categories was small, non-parametric statistics were applied to compare the components of ssbc (postural control, postural alignment and suck, swallow and breathing rhythm). the t-test with cohen’s d-values was used to determine the practical significance of the differences in the duration and frequency of feeding in the research and control groups. the chi-squared test was used to determine the statistical significance of differences between the research and control groups. cramer’s v-value was used to determine the effect size. the independent t-test and cronbach’s alpa test were used to determine the statistical significance and internal consistency of differences found in postural alignment and ssbc between the research and control groups. reliability and validity to ensure internal validity and reliability the participants in the research group were independently diagnosed by the family’s medical practitioner and the researcher was not part of the diagnostic procedure. all participants were assessed by the same clinician and all data entries were controlled by a second person. a second observer, blind to the presence or absence of infantile colic, was used to affirm the researcher’s observations. to enhance external validity, strict selection criteria were set and the assessment protocol was conducted during a scheduled feeding time. results top ↑ table 3a–c indicates the effect size of the difference in duration and frequency of feedings in the research and control groups of each age category. table 3a: effect size for the difference in duration and frequency of feedings in the research and control groups of each age category (age category 1: 2–4 weeks [n = 25]). table 3b: effect size for the difference in duration and frequency of feedings in the research and control groups of each age category (age category 2: 5–8 weeks [n = 25]). table 3b: effect size for the difference in duration and frequency of feedings in the research and control groups of each age category (age category 3: 9–12 weeks [n = 14]). the differences in duration and frequency of feedings of the research and control groups indicate medium and large effect sizes and a practical significance. feeding in participants with infantile colic took longer and was more frequent than in participants without the condition. table 4 provides a comparison of the results of the assessment protocol in the research and control groups in the age category 2–4 weeks. table 3c: effect size for the difference in duration and frequency of feedings in the research and control groups of each age category (age category 4: 13–19 weeks [n = 14]). almost all differences in postural control, postural alignment and suck, swallow and breathing rhythm (ssbr) between the two groups were significant (p < 0.05). it was only neck righting, grasp reflex, hand-to-mouth contact, pull to sit, supported standing and cup-shaped tongue that were not significant. a strong correlation between the postural control in prone and the presence of infantile colic was indicated by cramer’s v-value. participants with infantile colic took less weight on the shoulder girdle, had less neck extension and less hip flexion with pelvic elevation than expected for their age category on the assessment protocol. during postural adaptation for feeding the participants with colic did not assume a neutral cranio-cervical position and did not display hip flexion in one or both lower extremities. the infants with colic did not have a 1:1:1 ratio for suck, swallow and breathing or pausing between sucking cycles. table 4 indicates a correlation between postural control, postural alignment during feeding and ssbr and the presence of colic in participants in the age category 2–4 weeks. table 5 provides a comparison of the research and the control groups in the age category 5–8 weeks. table 4: comparison between participant groups in the category 1: 2–4 weeks (n = 26). statistically significant differences regarding postural control, postural alignment and ssbr were found. a correlation between the presence of colic and the absence of a neutral cranio-cervical position, quality of hip flexion and ratio of suck, swallow and breathing was found. cramer’s v-value indicated an effect size of greater than 0.5 for the findings for aspects of the above components of ssbc. during postural adaptation for feeding the participants with colic did not assume a neutral cranio-cervical position with a slightly curved back. they did not display hip flexion in one or both lower extremities and did not have a 1:1:1 ratio for suck, swallow and breathing with poor pausing between sucking cycles. table 5 indicates that the correlation between postural alignment during feeding and ssbr and the presence of colic was sustained in participants in the age category 5–8 weeks. table 6 gives a comparison of the results of the assessment protocol for the study and the control groups in the age category 9–12 weeks. table 5: comparison between participant groups in the category 2: 5–8 weeks (n = 24). again statistically significant differences regarding postural control were found between the study and control groups as well as a correlation with the presence of colic. all the aspects of postural alignment and ssbr were statistically significant with a strong correlation with the presence of colic. it appears that postural alignment and ssbr played an increased role in the presence of colic in the participants. table 6 also indicates a correlation between postural alignment, ssbr and the presence of colic in the age category 9–12 weeks. table 7 gives the results in the age category 13–19 weeks. table 6: comparison between participant groups in the category 3: 9–12 weeks (n = 14). once again statistically significant differences were indicated between postural control, postural alignment and ssbr of the study and control groups with a correlation between the presence of colic and the quality of postural control. cramer’s v-values indicate that the postural alignment and ssbr play an increasing role in the presence of colic. participants with colic had difficulty playing with hands to knees when in the supine position and rolling to the side. all participants in the control group were able to do so. the research group had difficulty pushing up on their elbows and shifting weight with the shoulder girdle. the research group also had difficulty with accidental rolling, stood with a wide base of support, had poor quality of supported sitting and showed difficulty when pulled to sit. again, table 7 indicates an even stronger correlation between postural alignment, ssbr and the presence of colic in the age category 13–19 weeks. the results, of all four age categories, indicate a large effect size and a correlation with the presence of infantile colic. table 8 displays the results of a t-test and a cronbach’s alpha validity coefficient. table 7: comparison between participant groups in the category 4: 13–19 weeks (n = 14). the results of the t-test indicate a statistically significant difference between the postural alignment and ssbr of all participants with and without colic. the cronbach’s alpha value indicates a good internal reliability for postural alignment across age categories. it is commonly accepted that poor postural control negatively impacts on postural alignment and disturbs feeding and swallowing (hall, 2001; redstone & west, 2004). this pattern is well-documented in infants with neurological difficulties (sheppard, 2008) but not in infants with colic. table 7 and table 8 indicate this same pattern of poor postural control with a negative impact on postural alignment resulting in a feeding disturbance. table 8: t-test results for postural alignment and ssbr in both participant groups. in the age category 2–4 weeks, five descriptors for postural control were found to be not significant (neck righting reaction, hand-to-hand or hand-to-mouth contact, grasp reflex, pull to sit and supported standing). in the age category 5–8 weeks, three descriptors for postural control and postural alignment were found to be not significant (presence of asymmetric tonic neck reflex, pull to sit and arm flexion to midline). in the age category 9–12 weeks, two descriptors for postural control were found to be not significant (supported sitting and supported standing). in the age category 13–19 weeks, only one descriptor for postural control was found to be not significant (ventral suspension). discussion top ↑ the main purpose of this study was to explore the ssbc in young infants with infantile colic. the participants with infantile colic took longer than the normal 20 minutes or less (arvedson & brodsky, 2002) to complete a feeding. they also fed more frequently with less than three hours between feeds. this may offer an explanation for the perception in general and in popular literature that infantile colic is associated with feeding difficulties. the finding also strengthens the rationale for exploring feeding difficulties in infants with infantile colic. an evaluation protocol for ssbc was compiled to clinically assess and compare a group of infants with and without the condition. the results indicate that ssbc can be assessed clinically and the assessment protocol could now be included in assessment and treatment planning for infants with colic. the results indicated that postural alignment and ssbr of participants with colic differed significantly from participants without the condition across age categories. the difficulties with postural control, postural alignment and ssbc appear to be subtle and present as feeding difficulty or infantile colic. redstone and west (2004) also indicate a correlation between the quality of postural alignment and the quality of feeding. the results highlight the importance of clinically assessing ssbc in infants with infantile colic in order to inform and influence clinical practice. the results are in agreement with miller-loncar et al. (2004), who also suggest that feeding difficulties are associated with infantile colic. the components of ssbc not statistically significant between the groups strengthen the importance of assessing ssbc clinically in infants with the condition. poor postural control and a negative impact on ssbc is found in infants with neurological difficulties (arvedson & lefton-greif, 1996; hall, 2001; lefton-greif & mcgrath-morrow, 2007; wolf & glass, 1992). although participants in the present study did not have neurological difficulties, a similar pattern emerged in the present study. with an increase in age, increasingly more aspects of postural control differed significantly between the research and control groups. literature indicates that infants develop more muscle control as reflexes diminish, enabling better postural adaptations for feeding (alexander et al., 1993; bly, 1995; redstone & west, 2004). the pattern of poor postural control impacted similarly, but in a subtle way, on postural alignment, resulting in a disturbance of ssbc. the feeding disturbance is much more subtle than in infants with neurological difficulties and may present as the symptoms parents describe for infantile colic. the findings suggest some truth in the perception that infantile colic is the result of feeding difficulties. infantile colic is further associated with the occurrence of communication-interaction difficulties between parent and infant, serious psychosocial difficulties, abuse and educational difficulties, which suggest a continuum of risk in infants with the condition. the results suggest the importance of a clinical assessment of ssbc and the involvement of a speech-language therapist for early feeding and communication intervention. although there appears to be some evidence of subtle disturbances in ssbc associated with infantile colic, the causes of colic still need to be investigated. due to the small sample size the findings of this study cannot be generalised. since a non-randomised convenient sampling method involving only two communities was used, bias may be present in the sample. it is recommended that further research should make use of larger sample sizes and involve more communities. conclusion top ↑ a need was identified to explore the importance of including ssbc as a possible contributing factor to infantile colic. considering the high prevalence (10%–40%) of infantile colic (deshpande, 2003), and by implication the risk for communication development delays, this article suggests that assessment of ssbc should be included in the diagnosis of infants with colic. literature provides well-documented treatment options for the components of ssbc (arvedson & brodsky, 2002; wolf & glass, 1992), which may improve the outcome of the condition in very young infants. speech-language therapists play an important role in the identification, intervention and outcome of feeding difficulties in young infants (asha, 2008). further research on the topic may expand the role of the speech-language therapists in early intervention. acknowledgements top ↑ competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions h.d. (north-west university) was responsible for performing the research, preparing the data for statistical analysis and writing the manuscript. a.k. (university of pretoria) supervised and reviewed the data collection, analysis and interpretation as well as the manuscript. references top ↑ alexander, r., boehme, r., & cupps, b. (1993). normal development of functional motor skills: the first year of life. tucson, az: therapy skill builders. arvedson, j.c., & brodsky, l. (2002). pediatric swallowing and feeding: assessment and management. san diego, ca: singular thomson learning. arvedson, j.c., & lefton-greif, m.a. (1996). anatomy, physiology and development of feeding. seminars in speech and language, 17, 261–279. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1064103 asha. (2008). roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists in early intervention: guidelines. retrieved july 2004, from www.asha.org/policy/gl2008-00293.htm bailey, s.d., d’auria, j.p., & haushalter, j.p. (2012). information on infantile colic on the world wide web. journal of pediatric health care, 27, 443–445. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pedhc.2012.04.009 barlow, s.m. (2009). central pattern generation involved in oral and respiratory control for feeding in the term infant. current opinion in otolaryngology & head and neck surgery, 17, 179–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/moo.0b013e32832b36fe barr, r.g., trent, r.b., & cross, j. (2006). age-related incidence curve of hospitalized shaken baby syndrome cases: convergent evidence for crying as a trigger to shaking. child abuse and neglect, 30, 7–16. beebe, s.a., casey, r., & pinto-martin, j. (1993). association of reported infant crying and maternal parenting stress. clinical pediatrics, 32, 15–19. berezin s., glassman m.s., bostwick h., & halata, m. (1995). esophagitis as a cause of infant colic. clinical pediatrics, 34(43), 158–159. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/000992289503400308 blanche, e.i., botticelli t.m., & hallway, m.k. (1995). combining neuro-developmental treatment and sensory integration principles: an approach to pediatric therapy. san antonio, tx: therapy skill builders. bly, l. (1995). motor skills acquisition in the first year of life. san antonio, tx: therapy skill builders. brown, l.f., thoyre, s., pridham, k., & schubert, c. (2009). the mother-infant feeding tool. journal of obstetric, gynecologic, & neonatal nursing, 38, 491–503. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1552-6909.2009.01047.x canivet, c., jakobsson, i., & hagander, b. (2000). infantile colic. follow-up at four years of age: still more “emotional”. acta paediatrica, 89(1), 13–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2000.tb01179.x catherine, n.l., ko, j.j., & barr, r.g. (2008). getting the word out: advice on crying and colic in popular parenting magazines. journal of developmental & behavioral pediatrics, 29, 508–511. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/dbp.0b013e31818d0c0c cohen-silver, j., & ratnapalan, s. (2009). management of infantile colic: a review. clinical pediatrics, 48(1), 14–17. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0009922808323116 cupps, b. (1997). postural control: a current view. ndta network, january/february, 1–7. deshpande, p.g. (2003). colic. retrieved july 2006, from www.emedicine.com/ped/topic434.hem finnie, n.r. (1992). handling your cerebral palsied child at home. london: william heinemann medical books. gomez, c.r., baird, s., & jung, l.a. (2004). regulatory disorder identification, diagnosis, and intervention planning: untapped resources for facilitating development. infants & young children, 17, 327–339. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00001163-200410000-00006 gudmundsson, g. (2010). infantile colic: is a pain syndrome. medical hypotheses, 75, 528–529. hall, k.d. (2001). pediatric dysphagia resource guide. clifton park, ny: singular publishing group. hall, b., chesters, j., & robinson, a. (2012). infantile colic: a systematic review of medical and conventional therapies. journal of pediatrics and child health, 48(2), 128–137. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1754.2011.02061.x harris, m. (1986). oral-motor management of the high risk neonate. pediatrics, 42, 231–253. http://dx.doi.org/10.1300/j006v06n03_12 heine, r.g. (2006). gastroesophageal reflux disease, colic and constipation in infants with food allergy. current opinion in allergy and clinical immunology, 6, 220–225. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.all.0000225164.06016.5d hemmi, m.h., wolke, d., & schneider, s. (2011). associations between problems with crying, sleeping and/or feeding in infancy and long-term behavioural outcomes in childhood: a meta-analysis. archives of disease in childhood, 96, 622–629. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2010.191312 kanabar, d. (2008). current treatment options in the management of infantile colic. prescriber, 19(7), 24–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/psb.220 kanabar, d., randhawa, m., & clayton, p. (2001). improvement of symptoms in infant colic following reduction of lactose load with lactase. journal of human nutrition and dietetics, 14, 359–363. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-277x.2001.00304.x kheir, a.e. (2012). infantile colic: facts and fiction. italian journal pediatrics, 38, 34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1824-7288-38-34 lefton-greif, m.a., & mcgrath-morrow, s.a. (2007). deglutition and respiration: development, coordination, and practical implications. seminars in speech and language, 28, 166–169. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2007-984723 lester, b.m., boukydis, c.z., garcia-coll, c.t., hole, w., & peucker, m. (1992). infantile colic: acoustic cry characteristics, maternal perception of cry, and temperament. infant behavior and development, 15(1), 15–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0163-6383(92)90003-o lucassen, p. (2010). colic in infants. bmj clinical evidence, 2010 (february). retrieved from http://www.clinicalevidence.com lucassen, p., assendelft, w., van eijk, j., gubbels, j., douwes, a., & van geldrop, w. (2001). systematic review of the occurrence of infantile colic in the community. archives of disease in childhood, 84, 398–403. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.84.5.398 mckirdy, l.s., sheppard, j.j., osborne, m.l., & payne, p. (2008). transition from tube to oral feeding in the school setting. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 39(april), 249–260. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/024) miller-loncar, c., bigsby, r., high, p., wallach, m., & lester, b. (2004). infant colic and feeding difficulties. archives of disease in childhood, 89, 908–912. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.2003.033233 morris, s.e, & klein, m.d. (2001). pre-feeding skills (2nd edn.): a comprehensive guide to mealtime development. tucson, az: therapy skill builders. oetter, p., richter, e.w., & frick, s.m. (1995). m.o.r.e integrating the mouth with sensory and postural functions. (2nd edn.). minnesota: pdp press. perkins, w.h., & kent, r.d. (1986). functional anatomy of speech, language, and hearing: a primer. boston, ma: allyn & bacon. räihä, h., lehtonen, l., huhtala, v., saleva, k., & korvenranta, h. (2002). excessively crying infant in the family: mother–infant, father–infant and mother–father interaction. child: care, health and development, 28, 419–429. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2214.2002.00292.x räihä, h., lehtonen, l., korhonen, t., & korvenranta, h. (1997). family functioning 3 years after infantile colic. journal of developmental & behavioral pediatrics, 18, 290–294. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00004703-199710000-00002 redstone, f., & west, j.f. (2004). the importance of postural control for feeding. pediatric nursing, 30(2), 97–100. reijneveld, s.a., brugman, e., & hirasing, r.a. (2000). infantile colic: maternal smoking as potential risk factor. archives of disease in childhood, 83, 302–303. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.83.4.302 rogers, b. (1996). neurodevelopmental presentation of dysphagia. seminars in speech and language, 17, 269–281. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-2008-1064104 rossetti, l.m. (2001). communication intervention: birth to three. san diego, ca: singular/thomson learning. savino, f. (2007). focus on infantile colic. acta paediatrica, 96, 1259–1264. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00428.x savino, f., cordisco, l., tarasco, v., palumeri, e., calabrese, r., oggero, r. et al. (2010). lactobacillus reuteri dsm 17938 in infantile colic: a randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. pediatrics, 126(3), e526–e533. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0433 savino, f., grassino, e.c., guidi, c., oggero, r., silvestro, l., & miniero, r. (2006). ghrelin and motilin concentration in colicky infants. acta paediatrica, 95, 738–741. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2006.tb02324.x savino, f., & tarasco, v. (2010). new treatments for infant colic. current opinion in pediatrics, 22, 791–797. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/mop.0b013e32833fac24 seikel, j.a., douglas w.k., & drumright, d.g. (2010). anatomy & physiology for speech, language, and hearing (4th edn.). clifton park, ny: delmar cengage learning. sheppard, j.j. (2008). using motor learning approaches for treating swallowing and feeding disorders: a review. language, speech, and hearing services in schools, 39, 227–236. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2008/022) søndergaard, c., skajaa, e., & henriksen, t.b. (2000). fetal growth and infantile colic. archives of disease in childhood-fetal and neonatal edition, 83(1), f44–f47. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/fn.83.1.f44 st james-roberts, i., conroy s., & wilsher, k. (1996). bases for maternal perception of infant crying and colic behaviour. archives of childhood disease, 75, 375–384. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.75.5.375 swigert, n.b. (2009). the source for pediatric dysphagia. east moline, il: linguisystems. vik, t., grote, v., escribano, j., socha, j., verduci, e., fritsch, m. et al. (2009). infantile colic, prolonged crying and maternal postnatal depression. acta paediatrica, 98, 1344–1348. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2009.01317.x wessel, m.a., cobb, j.c., jackson, e.b., harris, g.s., & detwiler, a.c. (1954). paroxysmal fussing in infancy, sometimes called “colic”. pediatrics, 14, 421–435. wolf, l.s, & glass, r.p. (1992). feeding and swallowing disorders in infancy: assessment and management. tucson, az: therapy skill builders. appendix a top ↑ assessment protocol for suck, swallow and breathing coordination (ssbc) the effects of electronic and other external control methods on stuttering: a review of some research techniques and suggestions for further research b y j o h n w f. bohr, b . a . h o n o r e s (pret.) s e n i o r r e s e a r c h officer, n a t i o n a l i n s t i t u t e for p e r s o n a l r e s e a r c h , s o u t h a f r i c a n c o u n c i l for s c i e n t i f i c a n d i n d u s t r i a l r e s e a r c h , j o h a n n e s b u r g . introduction d u r i n g t h e c o u r s e of a s e r i e s of s t u d i e s a i m e d a t d e t e r m i n i n g t h e r e l a t i o n s h i p b e t w e e n p e r f o r m a n c e u n d e r c o n d i t i o n s of d e l a y e d a u d i t o r y f e e d b a c k ( d a f ) a n d p e r f o r m a n c e o n t h r e e o t h e r t e s t s ( v i s u a l a n d a u d i t o r y r e a c t i o n t i m e s a n d t h e s t r o o p r e a d i n g t e s t ) , t h e a u t h o r ' s i n t e r e s t w a s a r o u s e d in t h e e x p e r i m e n t a l w o r k of c h e r r y , s a y e r s a n d m a r l u n d (5), o n t h e i n h i b i t i o n of s t u t t e r i n g b y m e a n s of e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l . t h e a u t h o r ' s e x p e r i m e n t a l w o r k in t h e field of d a f a m o n g 126 e n g l i s h s p e a k i n g s o u t h a f r i c a n u n i v e r s i t y s t u d e n t s , all of w h o m w e r e n o r m a l s p e a k e r s , i n d i c a t e d t h a t t h o s e s t u d e n t s w i t h a f o r m e r h i s t o r y of s t u t t e r i n g t y p i c a l l y s h o w e d a s m a l l e r d i s c r e p a n c y i n r e a d i n g t i m e s t h a n o t h e r s w i t h o u t s u c h h i s t o r y , w h e n p e r f o r m a n c e u n d e r t w o c o n d i t i o n s w e r e c o m p a r e d , v i z . r e a d i n g i). w i t h i m m e d i a t e a n d ii). w i t h d e l a y e d a u d i t o r y f e e d b a c k . i n n o r m a l s p e e c h t h e s u b j e c t c o n s t a n t l y m o n i t o r s h i s s p e e c h b y p e r c e p t i o n of h i s o u t p u t t h r o u g h b o t h e x t e r o c e p t i v e a n d p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s . d a f b r i n g s a b o u t a d i s r u p t i v e effect o n t h e f o r m e r , a n d m a s k s t h e t i s s u e b o r n e a u d i t o r y c u e s t o t h e h e a r i n g m e c h a n i s m , w i t h o u t a f f e c t i n g t h e r e m a i n d e r of p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s . i t m a y b e t h o u g h t t h a t t h e f u r t h e r d i m i n u t i o n of p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s w o u l d m a k e c o h e r e n t s p e e c h i m p o s s i b l e , b u t t h i s is n o t t h e c a s e , a s h a s b e e n d e m o n s t r a t e d b y l a d e f o g e d (9). h e e x c l u d e d e x t e r o c e p t i v e s p e e c h m o n i t o r i n g b y m e a n s of a l o u d m a s k i n g n o i s e in e a r p h o n e s w o r n b y t h e s u b j e c t , w h o s e p e r c e p t i o n of t i s s u e b o r n e a c o u s t i c c u e s w a s a l s o m a s k e d b y t h e l o u d n o i s e in t h e e a r p h o n e s . i n a n a t t e m p t t o d i m i n i s h o t h e r p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s t o t h e s u b j e c t , l a d e f o g e d h a d h i m s u c k a m e t h o c o c a i n e t a b l e t s , w h i c h p r o d u c e d l o c a l a n a e s t h e t i z a t i o n of t h e s u r f a c e of t h e l i p s , t o n g u e a n d m o u t h i n t e r i o r . e v e n u n d e r t h e s e c o n d i t i o n s of c o n s i d e r a b l e d i m i n u t i o n of b o t h e x t e r o c e p t i v e a n d p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s , t h e s u b j e c t w a s a b l e t o p r o d u c e i n t e l l i g i b l e s p e e c h . p r o b a b l y t h e s u b j e c t w a s s t i l l a b l e t o p e r c e i v e a l i m i t e d n u m b e r of r e d u c e d p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s c o n c e r n i n g m u s c l e s t r e t c h i n t h o s e a r e a s of t h e l i p s , t o n g u e a n d l a r y n x u n a f f e c t e d b y t h e a m e t h o c o c a i n e . 2. delayed auditory feedback (daf) w h e n a s p e a k e r is s u b j e c t e d t o d a f h e h e a r s h i s r e c o r d e d v o i c e a f r a c t i o n of a s e c o n d ( a b o u t 180 m i l l i s e c o n d s ) a f t e r u t t e r a n c e , t h r o u g h a h e a d s e t o r e a r p h o n e s . t h e s y s t e m is s o a r r a n g e d t h a t t h e d e l a y e d f e e d b a c k is a t a l e v e l h i g h e n o u g h t o m a s k t h e t i s s u e b o r n e a u d i t o r y c u e s t o t h e h e a r i n g m e c h a n i s m . t h u s t h e a u d i t o r y m o n i t o r i n g of s p e e c h is d i s r u p t e d , a l t h o u g h p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s r e m a i n u n a f f e c t e d . w i t h m o s t s u b j e c t s t h i s d i s r u p t i o n of n o r m a l a u d i t o r y s p e e c h m o n i t o r i n g p r o d u c e s v e r y m a r k e d c h a n g e s i n s p e e c h . l e e , w h o p u b l i s h e d t h e first p a p e r o n d a f (10), r e f e r s t o t h e s e effects a s " s t a r t l i n g " a n d s a y s of d a f : " . . . i t w i l l c a u s e t h e p e r s o n t o s t u t t e r , s l o w d o w n w h i l e r a i s i n g h i s v o i c e in p i t c h a n d v o l u m e , o r s t o p c o m p l e t e l y " . w o r k i n g in t h e s a m e field s i m u l t a n e o u s l y w i t h l e e , b u t i n d e p e n d e n t l y of h i m , b l a c k r e p o r t e d (1) t h a t d a f : " . . . m a y p r o d u c e n e a r t r a u m a t i c effects t h a t i n c l u d e t h e b l o c k i n g of s p e e c h , facial c o n t o r t i o n s , t h e p r o l o n g a t i o n a n d s l u r r i n g of s o u n d s , a n d r e p e t i t i o n s of s o u n d s a n d s y l l a b l e s . " b l a c k s t a t e s t h a t t h e d i s r u p t i v e eff e e t of d a f is m o s t m a r k e d w h e n t h e d e l a y t i m e of d a f is b e t w e e n 180 a n d 2 2 0 m i l l i s e c o n d s , w h i c h a p p r o x i m a t e s t h e m e a n s y l l a b l e d u r a t i o n for a v e r a g e s p e a k e r s . a p p a r a t u s for p r o d u c i n g d a f h a s b e e n d e s c r i b e d b y b l a c k (1 a n d 2), b o h r (3 a n d 4), f a i r b a n k s a n d j a e g e r (6), h a n l e y a n d t i f f a n y (7), m a r p l e a n d m o r r i l l (12), a n d t i f f a n y , h a n l e y a n d s u t h e r l a n d ( 1 4 ) . t y p i c a l l y , a m o d i f i e d m a g n e t i c t a p e r e c o r d e r is u s e d . t h e r e c o r d e r is f i t t e d w i t h t w o journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) h e a d s ( o n e for r e c o r d i n g a n d t h e o t h e r for s i m u l t a n e o u s p l a y b a c k of t h e r e c o r d e d s i g n a l ) a n d t h e o u t p u t f r o m t h e p l a y b a c k h e a d is fed i n t o a n a d d i t i o n a l p l a y b a c k a m p l i fier. t h e s u b j e c t h e a r s t h e o u t p u t f r o m t h e p l a y b a c k h e a d , s u i t a b l y a m p l i f i e d b y t h e p l a y b a c k a m p l i f i e r , t h r o u g h e a r p h o n e s o r a h e a d s e t . t h e d e g r e e of d e l a y i n t h e s y s t e m is d e p e n d e n t u p o n a n u m b e r of f a c t o r s , v i z . p h y s i c a l s e p a r a t i o n b e t w e e n r e c o r d i n g a n d p l a y b a c k h e a d s , s p e e d of t a p e t r a n s p o r t , a n d t h e e x i s t e n c e o r o t h e r w i s e of d e v i c e s for l e n g t h e n i n g t h e t a p e l o o p b e t w e e n t h e r e c o r d i n g a n d p l a y b a c k h e a d s . t h e s y s t e m d e v i s e d b y t h e a u t h o r is illus t r a t e d i n f i g u r e s 1 a n d 2. i n f i g u r e 1 t h e m o d i f i e d t a p e r e c o r d e r a p p e a r s i n t h e c e n t r e w i t h t h e p l a y b a c k a m p l i f i e r o n t h e r i g h t . t h e h e a d s e t s h o w n is p a r t of a l a b o r a t o r y t y p e a u d i o m e t e r ; for field u s e t h e a u t h o r e m p l o y s e i t h e r a s e t of e a r p h o n e s o r a s p e c i a l l y c o n s t r u c t e d p o r t a b l e h e a d s e t c o n t a i n i n g t w o i n v e r t e d c o n e l o u d s p e a k e r s . i n f i g u r e 2 t h e t w o h e a d s c a n b e s e e n in t h e l o w e r c e n t r e . t h e v a r i a b l e l o o p c o n t r o l l e r is s h o w n c e n t r e d b e t w e e n t h e t w o r e e l s , w h i l e t h e s e t of i n t e r c h a n g e a b l e c a p s t a n s a r e d i s p l a y e d a b o v e t h e l e f t h a n d t a p e r e e l . b y m e a n s of t h e i n t e r c h a n g e a b l e c a p s t a n s , w h i c h c o n t r o l t h e s p e e d of t a p e t r a n s p o r t , a n d t h e v a r i a b l e l o o p c o n t r o l l e r it is p o s s i b l e t o o b t a i n d e l a y t i m e s r a n g i n g f r o m 122 m i l l i s e c o n d s t o 8 9 7 m i l l i s e c o n d s . t h e p h y s i c a l s e p a r a t i o n b e t w e e n t h e t w o h e a d s is s u c h t h a t a d e l a y t i m e of 183 m i l l i s e c o n d s is o b t a i n e d w h e n t h e t a p e is t h r e a d e d s t r a i g h t t h r o u g h b e t w e e n h e a d s a n d t h e s p e e d of t a p e t r a n s p o r t is 1 \ i n c h e s p e r s e c o n d . fig. 1 d a f system devised b y author 1 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) fig. 2 modified tape recorder for daf 3. some south african results from daf experiments u s i n g t h e a p p a r a t u s d e s c r i b e d a b o v e , 126 e n g l i s h s p e a k i n g s e c o n d y e a r s t u d e n t s w i t h o u t c u r r e n t s p e e c h d e f e c t s w e r e t e s t e d a t t h e u n i v e r s i t i e s of c a p e t o w n , r h o d e s a n d n a t a l ( p i e t e r m a r i t z b u r g a n d d u r b a n ) . e a c h s u b j e c t w a s r e q u i r e d t o r e a d t h e g r a n d f a t h e r p a s s a g e t w i c e in s u c c e s s i o n . ( t h i s p a s s a g e b y v a n ' r i p e r (15) c o n t a i n s all e n g l i s h s p e e c h s o u n d s ) . f o r e a c h r e a d i n g t h e s u b j e c t w o r e a p o r t a b l e h e a d s e t . d u r i n g t h e first r e a d i n g h e h e a r d h i s v o i c e w i t h d i r e c t ( i m m e d i a t e ) f e e d b a c k t h r o u g h t h e h e a d s e t , a n d d u r i n g t h e s e c o n d r e a d i n g h e h e a r d , h i s v o i c e t h r o u g h t h e h e a d s e t w i t h a d e l a y t i m e of 183 m i l l i s e c o n d s . t h e r e s u l t s of t h i s t e s t s e r i e s a p p e a r i n t a b l e 1. t h e s e r e s u l t s r e v e a l t h a t d a f p r o d u c e s a m a r k e d r e d u c t i o n in r e a d i n g s p e e d for b o t h m a l e a n d f e m a l e s u b j e c t s . w h e n t h e m e a n s o n l y a r e c o n s i d e r e d , t h e r e is a c l o s e r r a t i o b e t w e e n t h e t w o r e a d i n g t i m e s for m a l e s t h a n for f e m a l e s . h o w e v e r , t h e r e is a g r e a t e r r a n g e o v e r w h i c h m a l e p e r f o r m a n c e s v a r y b o t h w i t h d i r e c t a n d d e l a y e d a u d i t o r y feedb a c k , a s s h o w n b y t h e l a r g e r s t a n d a r d d e v i a t i o n s for t h e m a l e s a m p l e u n d e r b o t h ' r e a d i n g c o n d i t i o n s . o n e of t h e f e m a l e s u b j e c t s r e p o r t e d h a v i n g a slight s t u t t e r a t a n e a r l i e r a g e . h e r r e a d i n g t i m e s w e r e 5 0 a n d 100 s e c o n d s for d i r e c t a n d d e l a y e d a u d i t o r y f e e d b a c k r e s p e c t i v e l y , g i v i n g a r a t i o of .50 b e t w e e n t h e t w o ' p e r f o r m a n c e s , w e l l below t h e m e a n s of b o t h f e m a l e a n d m a l e , g r o u p s . i n t h e m a l e s a m p l e t h e r e w e r e five s u b j e c t s w i t h a n e a r l i e r s t u t t e r i n g h i s t o r y . t h e i r p e r f o r m a n c e s a r e l i s t e d i n t a b l e 2. journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) table 1. reading times in seconds grandfather passage direct feedback daf „ ,. direct feedback ratio: daf 1. males mean 45.6* 61.8* .74 n = 58 standard deviation 6.3* 16.1* — 2. females mean 45.9* 65.1* .71 n = 68 standard deviation 5.0* 13.1* — * = significant beyond the .05% level. table 2. reading times (seconds), 5 male former stutterers subject direct feedback daf „ . direct feedback rat.o: daf a 43 33 1.303 β 45 56 .804 c i 46 48 .958 1 d 46 58 .793 ε 46 41 1.122 mean for group 45 sd=1.166 47 sd=9.325 .957 1 journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) if o n l y t h o s e m a l e s ( n = 5 3 ) w i t h n o r e p o r t e d s t u t t e r i n g h i s t o r y a r e c o n s i d e r e d t h e i r m e a n r e a d i n g t i m e s w i t h d i r e c t a n d d e l a y e d feedb a c k a r e 4 5 . 6 2 3 s e e s . ( s d = 6 . 5 6 ) a n d 6 3 . 1 3 2 s e e s . ( s d = 1 5 . 8 8 3 ) r e s p e c t i v e l y . a n f a n d t t e s t o n t h e r e s u l t s of n o n s t u t t e r e r s a n d f o r m e r s t u t t e r e r s r e v e a l e d a v e r y s i g n i f i c a n t d i f f e r e n c e ( . 0 0 1 ρ . 0 0 0 5 ) in t h e s t a n d a r d d e v i a t i o n s of t h e g r o u p s w i t h d i r e c t feedb a c k , b u t n o s i g n i f i c a n t d i f f e r e n c e b e t w e e n t h e m e a n s . u n d e r d a f , t h e s t a n d a r d d e v i a t i o n s of t h e g r o u p s d o n o t differ signific a n t l y , b u t t h e r e is a s i g n i f i c a n t d i f f e r e n c e b e t w e e n t h e m e a n s ( . 0 2 5 ρ .01). i n effect t h e 5 f o r m e r s t u t t e r e r s a r e l e s s a f f e c t e d i n r e a d i n g t i m e u n d e r d a f t h a n t h e 53 n o n s t u t t e r e r s . a l t h o u g h t h e s m a l l s a m p l e s i z e of f o r m e r s t u t t e r e r s p r e c l u d e s a n y final c o n c l u s i o n , t h e a u t h o r t e n t a t i v e l y i n f e r s t h a t t h e r e c o v e r e d s t u t t e r e r m a y h a v e d e v e l o p e d a t e c h n i q u e w h e r e b y h e t e n d s t o i g n o r e a u d i t o r y f e e d b a c k c u e s a n d t o r e l y m o r e h e a v i l y o n p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s for s p e e c h m o n i t o r i n g . if t h i s h y p o t h e s i s h o l d s , it p r e s e n t s t h e p o s s i b i l i t y of u t i l i z i n g d a f a s a p r o g n o s t i c m e a s u r e d u r i n g s p e e c h t h e r a p y . t h o s e s u b j e c t s s h o w i n g t h e s m a l l e s t d i s c r e p a n c y i n r e a d i n g t i m e s u n d e r d i r e c t a n d d e l a y e d a u d i t o r y f e e d b a c k c o u l d b e e x p e c t e d t o b e n e f i t m o s t f r o m s p e e c h t h e r a p y , a n d v i c e v e r s a . a l t e r n a t i v e l y d a f c o u l d b e e m p l o y e d a s a n i n d i c a t o r of t h e p r o g r e s s of t h e s t u t t e r e r d u r i n g s p e e c h t h e r a p y . i t s h o u l d b e e m p h a s i z e d t h a t t h e s e s u g g e s t i o n s a r e p u t f o r w a r d t e n t a t i v e l y . a t t h i s s t a g e , a n d s h o u l d b e c o n s i d e r e d a s s u g g e s t i o n s for f u r t h e r r e s e a r c h , w h i c h w i l l i n d i c a t e w h e t h e r t h e y a r e w a r r a n t e d . a f u r t h e r s u g g e s t i o n e m a n a t i n g f r o m t h e s e p r e l i m i n a r y f i n d i n g s is t h e c o n s i d e r a t i o n b y s p e e c h t h e r a p i s t s of e m p h a s i z i n g , d u r i n g t h e r a p y , t h a t s u b j e c t s s h o u l d a t t e m p t t o r e l y m o r e h e a v i l y d u r i n g s p e e c h o n p r o p r i o c e p t i v e c u e s a n d a t t e m p t t o i g n o r e t h e a u d i t o r y s p e e c h c u e s , w h i c h i n t h e c a s e of s t u t t e r e r s a c t u a l l y h a m p e r s p e e c h a n d t e n d t o m e d i a t e s t u t t e r i n g , a s s h o w n b y t h e w o r k of c h e r r y , s a y e r s a n d m a r l u n d (5). 5. an experiment using external control for stammering a n u m b e r of f o r m s of e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l t o d i s t r a c t t h e s t u t t e r e r f r o m t h e a u d i t o r y c u e s i n h i s o w n v o i c e h a v e b e e n p r o p o s e d b y c h e r r y , s a y e r s a n d m a r l u n d (5) for e x p e r i m e n t a l a n d c l i n i c a l u s e w i t h s t u t t e r e r s . t h e s e i n c l u d e s p e e c h s h a d o w i n g , s i m u l t a n e o u s r e a d i n g f r o m t h e s a m e t e x t , o r f r o m d i f f e r e n t t e x t s , g i b b e r i s h , w h i s p e r i n g , r e v e r s e d s p e e c h , d a f , w h i t e n o i s e m a s k i n g , l o w p i t c h m a s k i n g , e t c . , all of w h i c h t e n d t o p r o d u c e i m p r o v e d s p e e c h p r o d u c t i o n b y s t u t t e r e r s i n r o u t i n e r e a d i n g . b e f o r e d e a l i n g w i t h h i s e x p e r i m e n t s i n e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l w i t h a s t u t t e r e r , t h e a u t h o r w i s h e s t o p r e s e n t a b r i e f r e c a p i t u l a t i o n of e a c h of t h e s e t e c h n i q u e s : a . s p e e c h s h a d o w i n g . t h e e x p e r i m e n t e r r e a d s a l o u d o r s p e a k s e x t e m p o r e , a n d t h e s u b j e c t a t t e m p t s t o c o p y t h i s s p e e c h p a t t e r n a l o u d , b u t d o e s n o t s e e t h e t e x t ( w h e n u s e d ) . b. s i m u l t a n e o u s r e a d i n g . t h e e x p e r i m e n t e r r e a d s a l o u d , w h i l e t h e s u b j e c t a l s o r e a d s a l o u d f r o m t h e s a m e t e x t . c. u s e of different t e x t s . b o t h e x p e r i m e n t e r a n d s u b j e c t r e a d a l o u d f r o m d i f f e r e n t t e x t s . d. gibberish. e x p e r i m e n t e r a n d s u b j e c t s t a r t r e a d i n g a l o u d f r o m s a m e t e x t ; w h i l e t e s t is u n d e r w a y e x p e r i m e n t e r c h a n g e s w i t h o u t w a r n i n g i n t o m e a n i n g l e s s s o u n d s ( g i b b e r i s h ) . e. w h i s p e r i n g . t h e e x p e r i m e n t e r r e a d s a l o u d , w h i l e t h e s u b j e c t r e a d s t h e s a m e p a s s a g e i n a w h i s p e r . f. r e v e r s e d s p e e c h . t h e s u b j e c t r e a d s a l o u d , w h i l e a r e v e r s e d s p e e c h r e c o r d i n g is p l a y e d b a c k t o h i m t h r o u g h e a r p h o n e s f r o m a g r a m a p h o n e r e c o r d o r t a p e r e c o r d i n g . g. d a f . t h e s u b j e c t r e a d s , a l o u d , h i s v o i c e b e i n g r e c o r d e d o n m a g n e t i c t a p e a n d p l a y e d b a c k t o h i m t h r o u g h a h e a d s e t a f r a c t i o n of a s e c o n d a f t e r t h e u t t e r a n c e of e a c h s o u n d . h. w h i t e n o i s e m a s k i n g . t h e s u b j e c t r e a d s a l o u d , w h i l e a b r o a d b a n d of n o i s e e v e n l y d i s t r i b u t e d t h r o u g h o u t t h e a u d i o s p e c t r u m , a n d e l e c t r o n i c a l l y g e n e r a t e d , is fed i n t o t h e h e a d s e t h e is w e a r i n g . journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) i. l o w p i t c h m a s k i n g . t h e s u b j e c t r e a d s a l o u d , w h i l e a s i n e w a v e of a b o u t 140 c y c l e s p e r s e c o n d ( c p s ) is fed i n t o t h e h e a d s e t h e is w e a r i n g . a l l t h e a b o v e f o r m s of e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l c a n h a v e t h e effect of i m p r o v i n g t h e r e a d i n g of s t u t t e r e r s . i t c a n b e s e e n t h a t t h o s e f o r m s l i s t e d f r o m a . t o e . a b o v e c a n b e a p p l i e d w i t h o u t a n y e l e c t r o n i c d e v i c e s , w h i l s t t h o s e l i s t e d f r o m f. t o i. r e q u i r e s o m e w h a t e l a b o r a t e a p p a r a t u s . c h e r r y et.al. h a v e d e m o n s t r a t e d t h a t w h i t e n o i s e m a s k i n g c a n p r o d u c e a different i a l effect o n t h e i n h i b i t i o n of s t u t t e r i n g , d e p e n d i n g o n t h e p o r t i o n of t h e a u d i o s p e c t r u m r e a c h i n g t h e s u b j e c t : w h e n t h e w h i t e n o i s e is s u p p l i e d t o t h e s u b j e c t t h r o u g h a l o w p a s s filter w h i c h p a s s e s all a u d i b l e n o i s e b e l o w 5 0 0 c p s , t h e m e a n r e d u c t i o n i n b r e a k d o w n ( s t u t t e r i n g ) t i m e i s 9 5 % , p l u s o r m i n u s 5 % . w h e n t h e w h i t e n o i s e is s u p p l i e d t o t h e s u b j e c t t h r o u g h a h i g h p a s s f i l t e r w h i c h p a s s e s a u d i b l e n o i s e a b o v e 5 0 0 c p s , t h e m e a n r e d u c t i o n i n b r e a k d o w n ( s t u t t e r i n g ) t i m e is o n l y 3 5 % . p l u s o r m i n u s 3 7 % . t h u s w h i t e n o i s e b e l o w 5 0 0 c p s p r o d u c e s a g r e a t e r i m p r o v e m e n t i n s p e e c h p r o d u c t i o n t h a n t h a t a b o v e 5 0 0 c p s . m e t r o n o m e t e c h n i q u e of u n o b t r u s i v e l y a c c e n t u a t i n g s p e e c h r h y t h m w h e r e b y h e c a n r e d u c e h i s s t u t t e r u n d e r n o r m a l c i r c u m s t a n c e s . w h e n e x c i t e d , h e o m i t s t h e m e t r o n o m e h a n d s i g n a l s a n d s t u t t e r s m o r e t h a n u s u a l . h e u n d e r w e n t t h e e x p e r i m e n t a l s e r i e s o n t h e c l e a r l y s t a t e d u n d e r s t a n d i n g t h a t h e w a s p a r t i c i p a t i n g i n t h e r e p l i c a t i o n of a s e r i e s of e x p e r i m e n t s o r i g i n a l l y p e r f o r m e d i n l o n d o n , a n d w a s n o t u n d e r g o i n g a f o r m of t h e r a p y . t h r e e r e a d i n g p a s s a g e s w e r e u s e d i n t h i s s e r i e s : a r a n d o m l y s e l e c t e d p a s s a g e f r o m a p e r i o d i c a l a n d t w o s t a n d a r d r e a d i n g p a s s a g e s , e a c h c o n t a i n i n g all t h e s p e e c h s o u n d s i n e n g l i s h . t h e t w o s t a n d a r d p a s s a g e s w e r e v a n r i p e r ' s g r a n d f a t h e r p a s s a g e (16) a n d a r t h u r t h e y o u n g r a t , b y j o h n s o n , b r o w n , c u r t i s , e d n e y a n d k e a s t e r (8). t h r o u g h o u t t h e s e r i e s of t e s t s t h e s u b j e c t w o r e a p o r t a b l e h e a d s e t , t h r o u g h w h i c h h e r e c e i v e d all i n s t r u c t i o n s a n d e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l s i g n a l s . t h i s p r o c e d u r e w a s f o l l o w e d s o t h a t t h e w e a r i n g of t h e h e a d s e t d i d n o t in i t s e l f c o n s t i t u t e a n e x p e r i m e n t a l v a r i a b l e . t h e r e s u l t s of t h i s e x p e r i m e n t a l s e r i e s a r e p r e s e n t e d in t a b l e 3. table 3. reading times (seconds) with and without external controls test sequence reading passage circumstances of test reading time 1. grandfather subject reads without external control 200 2. speech shadowing 63 3. simultaneous reading with experimenter 50 subject whispers. experimenter reads aloud 45 delayed auditory feedback (daf) 55 6. immediate auditory feedback 55 9. white noise masking at 110 db 60 10. 11. white noise masking at 117 db low pitch (140 cps) masking at 97 db~ 48 51 8. arthur the young rat subject reads without external control 96 12. low-pitch (140 cps) masking at 97 db 49 13. low-pitch (140 cps)masking at 97 db switched rapidly on and off 15. subject reads without external control 54 1 0 8 7. from a periodical subject reads without external control 200 14. subject reads without external control 155 t h e a u t h o r r e p l i c a t e d s o m e of t h e e x p e r i m e n t s i n , e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l w i t h a m a l e s t u d e n t , agetf 2 0 y e a r s . n o r m a l l y t h e s u b j e c t h a s n o d i f f i c u l t y i n s p e a k i n g i n t h e p r e s e n c e of a v e r y y o u n g c h i l d o r a p e t a n i m a l , a n d h e c a n s i n g o r c h e e r i n a g r o u p . w h e n i n t h e c o m p a n y of h i s p e e r s o r s u p e r i o r s , h o w e v e r , h e s t u t t e r s s e v e r e l y . h e h a s d e v e l o p e d a t h e r e s u l t s s h o w c e r t a i n a n o m a l i e s . f i r s t l y t h e r e is a c l e a r i n d i c a t i o n t h a t e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l w a s a s s o c i a t e d w i t h i m p r o v e d r e a d i n g . t h i s c a n b e s e e n f r o m t h e f i r s t s e r i e s (i.e. t e s t s 2, 3, 4 , 5, 9, 10 a n d 11) a s w e l l a s f r o m t h e s e c o n d s e r i e s ( t e s t s 12 a n d 13). s e c o n d l y r e a d i n g p e r f o r m a n c e a l s o i m p r o v e d when no external control w a s applied (tests 1 journal of the south african logopedic society 9 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 a n d 14). t h i r d l y i t a p p e a r s a s if t h e r e is a s e q u e n t i a l i m p r o v e m e n t i n r e a d i n g d u r i n g t h e c o u r s e of t h e t e s t s e r i e s , a s is c l e a r l y d e m o n s t r a t e d w h e n t h e t h i r d s e r i e s is e x a m i n e d . in t e s t s 7 a n d 14 n o e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l w a s a p p l i e d , b u t t h e p e r f o r m a n c e o n t e s t 14 w a s a c o n s i d e r a b l e i m p r o v e m e n t o n t h a t of t e s t 7. h o w e v e r , t h i s s e q u e n t i a l i m p r o v e m e n t d o e s n o t h o l d c o n s i s t e n t l y for t h e first s e r i e s ( t e s t s 1, 2, 3, 4, 5, 6, 9, 10 a n d 11) o r for t h e s e c o n d s e r i e s ( t e s t s 8, 12, 13 a n d 15), i n f a c t t h e p o o r e s t p e r f o r m a n c e s e q u e n t i a l l y w a s o n t e s t 15, w h e n c o m p a r e d w i t h t e s t 8. i t c o u l d b e a r g u e d t h a t f a t i g u e s e t in a t t e s t 15, a s a s u d d e n s t e p f u n c t i o n , a s t e s t 14 i m m e d i a t e l y p r e c e d i n g i t r e p r e s e n t e d a 2 2 % r e d u c t i o n in r e a d i n g t i m e o v e r t e s t 7. t o s u m u p t h e r e s u l t s of t h i s s e r i e s of e x p e r i m e n t s , it m a y b e s t a t e d v e r y t e n t a t i v e l y t h a t t h e r e a p p e a r s t o b e a t r e n d for i m p r o v e d r e a d i n g t o b e a s s o c i a t e d w i t h e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l . t h e s e e x p e r i m e n t s c e r t a i n l y d o n o t w a r r a n t t h e c o n c l u s i o n t h a t e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l p e r s e r e d u c e s s t u t t e r i n g . i t is a p p a r e n t t h a t c o n s i d e r a b l e f u r t h e r r e s e a r c h , u s i n g a l a r g e r s a m p l e a s w e l l a s c o n t r o l a n d e x p e r i m e n t a l c o n d i t i o n s a l t e r n a t e l y , is r e q u i r e d a field in w h i c h s p e e c h t h e r a p i s t s c o u l d p l a y a n i m p o r t a n t p a r t . 6. external control and stuttering r e s u l t s r e p o r t e d b y c h e r r y , s a y e r s a n d m a r l u n d (5) a n d m c l a r e n (11) i n d i c a t e t h a t c o n s i d e r a b l e s u c c e s s h a s a l r e a d y b e e n a c h i e v e d w h e n e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l is u s e d i n c o n j u n c t i o n w i t h o t h e r t e c h n i q u e s in t h e t r e a t m e n t of s t u t t e r e r s . m c l a r e n (11) e m p h a s i z e s t h a t n o o n e t e c h n i q u e c a n b e t h e i d e a l a p p r o a c h t o t h e p r o b l e m , a s s t u t t e r i n g is a n i n f i n i t e l y c o m p l e x p h e n o m e n o n , i n v o l v i n g s p e e c h i n t h e a c t of c o m m u n i c a t i o n a n d is i n t i m a t e l y a s s o c i a t e d w i t h h u m a n r e l a t i o n s h i p s . o v e r a p e r i o d of 3 y e a r s e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l h a s b e e n u s e d a t s t . m a r y ' s h o s p i t a l , l o n d o n , t o b u i l d u p a d y n a m i c t e c h n i q u e , a d a p t a b l e t o all a g e s a n d t y p e s of s t u t t e r e r s . m a c l a r e n (11, p . 4 6 0 ) s t a t e s : " t h e y o u n g p a t i e n t p r o v i d e d t h a t e n v i r o n m e n t a l s t r e s s e s c a n b e r e l i e v e d r e s p o n d s w e l l t o d i r e c t a u d i t o r y r e t r a i n i n g . " w i t h t h e o l d e r p a t i e n t t h e a n x i e t y a t t a c h e d t o t h e s p e e c h s i t u a t i o n o f t e n s p r e a d s i t s t e n t a c l e s t h r o u g h t h e w h o l e p a t t e r n of life. i t w o u l d b e a b s u r d t o e x p e c t t h a t a u d i t o r y r e t r a i n i n g a l o n e c o u l d b r i n g a n y r e a l relief t o s u c h p a t i e n t s . b u t h e r e o u r t e c h n i q u e h a s b e e n f o u n d t o b e of g r e a t v a l u e , for i t n o t o n l y s e r v e s t o s t r e n g t h e n a n d r e i n f o r c e t h e m e m o r y of n o r m a l i t y b u t it c a n b e u s e d a s a n i n s t r u m e n t b y w h i c h , in a v e r y p r a c t i c a l w a y , t h e s t a m m e r e r c a n b e b r o u g h t t o r e a l i s e i n a c t u a l e x p e r i e n c e t h e n a t u r e of h i s d i f f i c u l t i e s a n d t h e p r i n c i p l e s w h i c h m u s t b e f o l l o w e d if h e is t o f r e e h i m s e l f f r o m t h e m . " t h e r e l a t i o n s h i p b e t w e e n s t u t t e r i n g a n d e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l a w a i t s full e l u c i d a t i o n . m e a n w h i l e t h e h y p o t h e s i s p r o p o s e d b y c h e r r y , s a y e r s a n d m a r l u n d (5) h a s m u c h t o c o m m e n d it. a s t h e y s e e it, s t u t t e r i n g i s m a i n l y m e d i a t e d b y t h e s u b j e c t ' s a b n o r m a l p e r c e p t i o n of t h e l o w f r e q u e n c y c o m p o n e n t s of h i s o w n v o i c e , p a r t i c u l a r l y t h e b o n e a n d t i s s u e c o n d u c t e d l a r y n x t o n e s . t h e y d o n o t s u g g e s t t h a t t h i s a b n o r m a l p e r c e p t i o n is a cause of s t u t t e r i n g . t h e r o l e of e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l is t h e n t o d i m i n i s h t h e s t u t t e r e r ' s a w a r e n e s s of t h e l o w f r e q u e n c y l a r y n g e a l s t i m u l i b y m e a n s of m a s k i n g . if t h e s t u t t e r e r ' s b a s i l a r m e m b r a n e s a r e b r o u g h t i n t o v i b r a t i o n b y m e a n s of a l o u d t o n e in t h e r e g i o n of 140 c p s , all o t h e r h e a r i n g s t i m u l i a r e m a s k e d w i t h t h e r e s u l t t h a t t h e p e r c e p t u a l a b n o r m a l i t y is e f f e c t i v e l y b l o c k e d a n d s t u t t e r i n g is d i m i n i s h e d . 7. suggested further research on external control o n t h e b a s i s of t h e w o r k a l r e a d y d o n e i n b r i t a i n b y c h e r r y , s a y e r s a n d m a r l u n d a n d t h e s o m e w h a t i n c o n c l u s i v e r e s u l t s f r o m s o u t h a f r i c a n r e p l i c a t i o n of s o m e of t h e i r t e c h n i q u e s of e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l , t h e a u t h o r / c o n s i d e r s t h a t e x h a u s t i v e r e s e a r c h i n t h i s field p r o m i s e s w e l l for t h e d e v e l o p m e n t of p r a c t i c a l t e c h n i q u e s for t h e a l l e v i a t i o n of s t u t t e r i n g . t h e a u t h o r p o s e d t h e p r o b l e m of d e s i g n i n g a s m a l l , p o r t a b l e t r a n s i s t o r o s c i l l a t o r c a p a b l e of p r o d u c i n g a s t a b l e w a v e f o r m i n t h e v i c i n i t y of 140 c p s t o t h e a c o u s t i c r e s e a r c h l a b o r a t o r y of t h e c o u n c i l for s c i e n tific a n d i n d u s t r i a l r e s e a r c h . d r . j . f. b u r g e r 10 journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) d e s i g n e d a n d c o n s t r u c t e d a p r o t o t y p e m u l t i v i b r a t o r u s i n g t w o t r a n s i s t o r s t o p r o d u c e a v e r y s t a b l e w a v e f o r m , t h e f r e q u e n c y of w h i c h c a n b e v a r i e d v e r y s i m p l y b y c o n t r o l l i n g t h e p o w e r s u p p l y v o l t a g e . t h e r a n g e of f r e q u e n c y is f r o m 123 t o 165 c p s , w h i c h is t h o u g h t t o b e w i d e e n o u g h t o a c c o m o d a t e i n d i v i d u a l d i f f e r e n c e s in r e s o n a n t f r e q u e n c y of t h e b a s i l a r m e m b r a n e s of d i f f e r e n t s u b j e c t s . f i g u r e 3 s h o w s t h e c i r c u i t d i a g r a m of t h i s m u l t i v i b r a t o r , t o g e t h e r w i t h t h e w a v e f o r m p r o d u c e d a n d d e t a i l s of c h a n g e s i n f r e q u e n c i e s a s s o c i a t e d w i t h c h a n g e s in p o w e r s u p p l y v o l t a g e . f i g u r e 3 circuit diagram, multi-vibrator p o w e r s u p p l y mb. t h e f r e q u e n c y o f t h e m u l t i v i b r a t o r v a r i e s a c c o r d i n g t o t h e p o w e r s u p p l y v o l t a g e , a 3 s h o w n b e l o w . p o w e r supply f v o l t s ) 3 -4 5 6 9 frequency ( c y c l e s p e r s e c o w d ) 165 14-6 i3e i 23 wave form produced t i m e α b s 1-7 time ukjit6 b c • 2ό time units a -c «3·τ time units" owe complete cvclb journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t o d a t e , n o e x p e r i m e n t s w i t h s t u t t e r e r s h a v e b e e n p e r f o r m e d u s i n g t h i s m u l t i v i b r a t o r . h o w e v e r , it is s u g g e s t e d t h a t i t c o u l d b e l i n k e d w i t h a t r a n s i s t o r p o w e r a m p l i f i e r , m i c r o p h o n e , s w i t c h i n g s y s t e m a n d e a r p h o n e s i n a s m a l l p o r t a b l e s e t t h a t c o u l d b e w o r n b y a s t u t t e r e r o v e r a n e x t e n d e d p e r i o d . t h e p r o p o s e d s y s t e m is s h o w n in f i g u r e 4. t h e c i r c u i t h a s b e e n s o d e s i g n e d t h a t o u t p u t f r o m e i t h e r t h e m u l t i v i b r a t o r o r t h e m i c r o p h o n e c a n b e fed t o t h e e a r p h o n e s a f t e r a m p l i f i c a t i o n . t h u s , w h e n t h e s t u t t e r e r is l i s t e n i n g , h e s w i t c h e s t h e m i c r o p h o n e i n t o a n d t h e m u l t i v i b r a t o r o u t of c i r c u i t . f i g u r e ablock: diagram, multi-vibrator microphone, amplifier switching m i c r o p h o n e 1 journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) w h e n h e w i s h e s t o s p e a k , h e s w i t c h e s t h e m i c r o p h o n e o u t of a n d t h e m u l t i v i b r a t o r int o t h e c i r c u i t . t h i s t h e n p r o d u c e s t h e c h a r a c t e r i s t i c s q u e a l in t h e e a r p h o n e s s e t t i n g h i s b a s i l a r m e m b r a n e s i n t o v i b r a t i o n a n d o b l i t e r a t i n g all p e r c e p t i o n of v o i c e t o n e s . f o r c o s m e t i c r e a s o n s t h e e a r p h o n e s c o u l d b e of t h e h e a r i n g a i d t y p e . s h o u l d t h i s p r o p o s e d s y s t e m e v o k e t h e d e s i r e d effect of i n h i b i t i n g s t u t t e r i n g t h r o u g h t h e m a s k i n g of t h e s t u t t e r e r ' s p e r c e p t i o n of l o w f r e q u e n c y c o m p o n e n t s of h i s v o i c e , it is f u r t h e r s u g g e s t e d t h a t t h e r a p y c o u l d p r o c e e d a l o n g t h e l i n e s t h e h u l l i a n l e a r n i n g t h e o r y of r e i n f o r c e m e n t , i m m e d i a t e r e w a r d , m a s s i n g of r e i n f o r c e m e n t , r e a c t i v e / i n h i b i t i o n a n d c o n t r o l l e d i n h i b i t i o n a s p r o p o s e d b y w a l t o n a n d b l a c k ( 1 5 ) . i n effect , t h e n t h e e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l w o u l d c o n s t i t u t e a ' t e m p o r a r y " c r u t c h " t o t h e s t u t t e r e r d u r i n g t h e p r o c e s s of a u d i t o r y r e c o n d i t i o n i n g . w h i l e t h i s p a p e r w a s in p r e p a r a t i o n , t h e a u t h o r ' s a t t e n t i o n w a s d r a w n t o t h e . v o r k of p a r k e r a n d c h r i s t o p h e r s o n (8), of w h i t t i n g h a m h o s p i t a l , n r . p r e s t o n , l a n c a s h i r e . u s i n g a s m a l l t r a n s i s t o r t o n e p r o d u c i n g a p p a r a t u s w h i c h t h e p a t i e n t c o u l d s w i t c n off o r o n a t w i l l , w h e n d e s i r i n g t o h e a r o r s p e a k , t h e y s u c c e s s f u l l y t r e a t e d t h r e e s t u t t e r e r s , t w o of w h o m h a d r e c e i v e d n o b e n e f i t f r o m c o n v e n t i o n a l s p e e c h t h e r a p y . f i n a l l y , t h e a u t h o r ' s a t t e n t i o n h a s b e e n d r a w n t o t h e a s y e t u n p u b l i s h e d w o r k of d r . c . d . r o o d e ( d e p t . , of p s y c h o l o g y , u n i v e r s i t y of p o t c h e f s t r o o m ) o n e a r d o m i n a n c e a n d c e r e b r a l s p e e c h l o c a l i z a t i o n , a t t h e u n i v e r s i t y of o t t a w a . i n t h e l i g h t of t h i s w o r k it w o u l d a p p e a r t h a t r e s e a r c h i n t o e a r d o m i n a n c e m a y w e l l b e a m a j o r f a c t o r in e l u c i d a t i n g t h e a e t i o l o g y a n d t r e a t m e n t of s p e e c h d e f e c t s . summary s o m e r e s u l t s f r o m s o u t h a f r i c a n e x p e r i m e n t s w i t h d e l a y e d a u d i t o r y f e e d b a c k a r e p r e s e n t e d , f r o m w h i c h it is a p p a r e n t t h a t r e a d i n g p e r f o r m a n c e of m a l e f o r m e r s t u t t e r e r s differs from, t h a t of m a l e s w i t h n o r e p o r t e d h i s t o r y of s t u t t e r i n g . t h e c h e r r y s a y e r s m a r l u n d e x p e r i m e n t s i n t h e field of e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l s a n d s t u t t e r i n g a r e r e v i e w e d a n d s o m e r e s u l t s f r o m s o u t h a f r i c a n r e p l i c a t i o n of s o m e of t h e s e e x p e r i m e n t s w i t h a m a l e s t u t t e r e r a r e p r e s e n t e d . a t r a n s i s t o r m u l t i v i b r a t o r w i t h i n t e r e s t i n g r e s e a r c h a n d c l i n i c a l p o s s i b i l i t i e s is d e s c r i b e d . r e c e n t w o r k in b r i t a i n a n d c a n a d a is m e n t i o n e d a n d s o m e s u g g e s t i o n s for f u t u r e r e s e a r c h a r e m a d e . opsomming r e s u l t a t e v a n s u i d a f r i k a a n s e p r o e w e o p d i e g e b i e d v a n v e r t r a a g d e a k o e s t i e s e t e r u g v o e r i n g w o r d a a n g e b i e d , w a a r u i t b l y k d a t d a a r v e r s k i l l e in l e e s p r e s t a s i e b e s t a a n t u s s e n m a n l i k e g e w e s e h a k k e l a a r s e n m a n s m e t g e e n a a n g e m e l d e h a k k e l g e s k i e d e n i s . d i e c h e r r y s a y e r s m a r l u n d p r o e w e t e n o p s i g t e v a n e k s t e r n e k o n t r o l e s e n h a k k e l w o r d g e m e l d t e s a a m m e t n a v o r s i n g s g e g e w e n s u i t d i e h e r h a l i n g v a n s o m m i g e v a n h i e r d i e p r o e w e m e t ' n m a n l i k e h a k k e l a a r i n s u i d a f r i k a . ' n t r a n s i s t o r m u l t i v i b r a t o r m e t i n t e r e s s a n t e n a v o r s i n g s e n k l i n i e s e m o o n t l i k h e d e w o r d b e s k r y w e . o n l a n g s e w e r k i n b r i t t a n j e e n k a n a d a w o r d g e n o e m e n v o o r s t e l l e v i r t o e k o m s t i g e n a v o r s i n g w o r d v o o r g e l e . acknowledgements many persons and institutions made the present paper possible. whilst expressing his thanks to them, the author emphasizes that the views stated are his own and do not represent those of any person or institution mentioned below: the national council for social research of the department of education, arts and science gave financial assistance. dr. s. biesheuvel, former director of the national institute for personnel research and his successor, dr. d. j. gouws made apparatus and facilities available. the study was conducted for degree purpose at the university of pretoria under the joint guidance of prof. d. j. swiegers and dr. h. reuning. many members of the nipr assisted at various stages: mostly as patient and forebearing subjects. mr. d. r. de wet assisted in the design of the equipment, miss s. kaan and messrs. α. ο. h. roberts and j. shepherd advised on and assisted with statistical processing. the daf apparatus was modified by mr. t. dicker to the design of mr. j. kidd, both of the csir central workshops. mr. w. keet of the csir acoustic research laboratory assisted with initial daf experiments and dr. j. f. burger of the same laboratory designed and constructed the proto-type multi-vibrator. mr. r. serdyn of the csir took the photographs and miss e. woudstra of the nipr prepared the diagrams. professors e. pratt-yule, e. wild, c. danziger and r. albino and their colleagues and students made facilities available and co-operated in the experiments at the universities of cape town, rhodes (grahamstown) and natal (pietermaritzburg and durban). 1 journal of the south african logopedic society 13 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 9. b i b l i o g r a p h y 1. b l a c k , j . w . t h e effect of d e l a y e d s i d e t o n e u p o n v o c a l r a t e a n d i n t e n s i t y . j. s p e e c h a n d h e a r i n g d i s o r d e r s , 16, 5 6 6 0 . 1951. 2. b l a c k , j . w . t h e p e r s i s t e n c e of t h e e f f e c t s of d e l a y e d s i d e t o n e . j. s p e e c h a n d h e a r i n g d i s o r d e r s , 20, 6 5 6 8 . , 1955. 3. b o h r , j . w . f . a t e c h n i c a l n o t e o n t h e d e s i g n a n d c o n s t r u c t i o n of a n a p p a r a t u s for p r o d u c i n g d e l a y e d a u d i t o r y f e e d b a c k . p a p e r p r e s e n t e d t o t h e x i h t h a n n u a l c o n g r e s s , s o u t h a f r i c a n p s y c h o l o g i c a l a s s o c i a t i o n , s t e l l e n b o s c h , 1961, 8p. 4 . b o h r , j . w . f . v e r t r a a g d e a k o e s t i e s e t e r u g v o e r i n g b y p e r s o n e m e t e n s o n d e r s p r a a k g e b r e k e . referaat gelewer voor d e p a r t e m e n t e l e s i e l k u n d i g e s , s k o o l k l i n i e k , t r a n s v a a l s e o n d e r w y s d e p a r t e m e n t , pretoria, a p r i l 1963. 15p. 5. c h e r r y , c., s a y e r s , b. m c a . & m a r l u n d , p a u l i n e . e x p e r i m e n t s u p o n t h e t o t a l i n h i b i t i o n of s t a m m e r i n g b y e x t e r n a l c o n t r o l a n d s o m e c l i n i c a l r e s u l t s . j. p s y c h o s o m . r e s . , 1956, 1, 2 2 3 2 4 6 ; r e p r i n t e d in e y s e n c k , h . j . ( e d i t o r ) . b e h a v i o u r t h e o r y a n d t h e n e u r o s e s . o x f o r d : p e r g a m o n p r e s s , i 9 6 0 , 4 4 1 4 5 6 . 6. f a i r b a n k s , g . & j a e g e r , r. a d e v i c e for c o n t i n u o u s l y v a r i a b l e t i m e d e l a y of h e a d s e t m o n i t o r i n g d u r i n g m a g n e t i c r e c o r d i n g of s p e e c h . j. s p e e c h a n d h e a r i n g d i s o r d e r s , 16, 162-164. 1951. 7. h a n l e y , c. n . & t i f f a n y , w . r. a n inv e s t i g a t i o n i n t o t h e u s e of e l e c t r o m e c h a n i c a l l y d e l a y e d s i d e t o n e i n a u d i t o r y t e s t i n g . j. s p e e c h a n d h e a r i n g d i s o r d e r s , 19, 3 6 7 3 7 4 . 1954. 8. j o h n s o n , w . , b r o w n , c. f., c u r t i s , j . f . , e d n e y , c. w . & k e a s t e r , j. s p e e c h h a n d i c a p p e d children, n e w y o r k : h a r p e r , p . 4 2 2 , 1948. 9. l a d e f o g e d , p . t h e p e r c e p t i o n of s p e e c h . proc. s y m p o s i u m o n m e c h a n i z a t i o n a n d t h o u g h t p r o c e s s e s , 3 2 0 3 3 3 , 1958. 10. l e e , b. s. s o m e e f f e c t s of s i d e t o n e d e l a y . j. a c o u s t . s o c . a m e r . , 22, 6 3 9 6 4 0 . 1950. 11. m a c l a r e n , j . t h e t r e a t m e n t of s t a m m e r i n g b y t h e c h e r r y s a y e r s m e t h o d . in e y s e n c k ( s e e 5. a b o v e ) , 4 4 1 4 5 6 . 12. m a r p l e , ν . b. & m o r r i l l , s. n . a d e v i c e for t h e p r o d u c t i o n of d e l a y e d s i d e t o n e . , j o i n t p r o j e c t , n m 0 0 1 0 6 4 . 0 1 . 0 8 , r e p o r t n o . 8 p e n s a c o l a , o h i o s t a t e u n i v e r s i t y . 13. p a r k e r , c. s. & c h r i s t o p h e r s o n , f. e l e c | ' t r o n i c a i d i n t h e t r e a t m e n t of s t a m m e r . m e d . e l e c t r o n . biol. e n g n g . , v o l . 1. n o . l . 1 2 1 1 2 5 . 1963. 14. tiffany, w . r., h a n l e y , c. n . & s u t h e r l a n d , l. c. a s i m p l e m e c h a n i c a l a d a p t e r for v a r i a b l e s i d e t o n e d e l a y . j. s p e e c h a n d h e a r i n g d i s o r d e r s , 115, 5 9 6 0 . 1954. 15. v a n r i p e r , c. s p e e c h c o r r e c t i o n princ i p l e s a n d m e t h o d s . n e w y o r k : p r e n t i c e h a l l , 1 6 3 -1 6 4 , 1939. 16. w a l t o n , d . & b l a c k , d . a. t h e a p p l i c a t i o n of l e a r n i n g t h e o r y t o t h e t r e a t m e n t of s t a m m e r i n g . j. p s y c h o s o m . r e s . 3, 170-179; 1 9 5 8 a l s o r e p r i n t e d in e y s e n c k , h . ( s e e 5 a b o v e ) , 123-134. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) knowledge and attitudes of teachers regarding the impact of classroom acoustics on speech perception and learning knowledge and attitudes of teachers regarding the impact of classroom acoustics on speech perception and learning lebogang ramma university of cape town abstract this study investigated the knowledge and attitude of primary school teachers regarding the impact of poor classroom acoustics on learners’ speech perception and learning in class. classrooms with excessive background noise and reflective surfaces could be a bar­ rier to learning, and it is important that teachers are aware of this. there is currently limited research data about teachers’ knowledge regarding the topic of classroom acoustics. seventy teachers from three johannesburg primary schools participated in this study. a sur­ vey by way of structured self-administered questionnaire was the primary data collection method. the findings of this study showed that most of the participants in this study did not have adequate knowledge of classroom acoustics. most of the participants were also un­ aware of the impact that classrooms with poor acoustic environments can have on speech perception and learning. these results are discussed in relation to the practical implication of empowering teachers to manage the acoustic environment of their classrooms, limi­ tations of the study as well as implications for future research. k eyw ord s: classroom acoustics; background noise, reverberation, teacher's voice, s chool classrooms with poor acoustic environment (i.e. excessive background noise levels and too much rever­ beration) are not suitable for educational activities that requires listening (anderson, 2004; crandell & smaldino, 2000; dockrell, shield & rigby, 2004). excessive background noise in a classroom makes the communication between the teacher and i learners difficult because it can cover up part of the verbal mes ✓ ' i -ysage conveyed by the teacher to learners and vice versa (crandell & smaldino, 1994). similarly, too many reflective surfaces in the classroom (hence too much reverberation) can cause speech i sounds communicated by the teacher to the learners to bounce around the room, leading |to blurring of the final speech signal heard by the learners (guckelberger, 2003). this will make it hard for some learners to hear and understand what is being communicated to them due to poor speech perception. speech perception in this study refers to the process of hearing and un­ derstanding speech signals during normal human communication (massaro, 2001). poor classroom acoustics as a barrier to learning primary school children are more vulnerable to the effects of extraneous noise sources (shield & dockrell, 2004). therefore, classrooms with too much background noise and reverberation can have a negative impact on learners' speech perception, and i consequently their learning (anderson, 2004). several studies that investigated the impact of too much background noise in the classroom on learners’ ability to learn have shown marked nega­ tive effects of noise on learners’ reading and numeracy skills, as well as on overall academic performance (lundquist, holmberg & landstrom, 2000; mackenzie, 2000; maxwell & evans, 2000; shield, & dockrell, asker & tachmatzidis, 2002). meaningful irrelevant speech, such as noise from people speaking in an adja­ cent classroom, has been shown to affect speech understanding, and or learning, to a higher degree than other types of noises (boman, enmarker & hygge, 2005). poor acoustics in the class­ room have also been shown to have the following effects on learners: high levels of listening fatigue (hicks & tharpe, 2002); poor attentive behaviour, especially when the subject matter is complex or unfamiliar (anderson, 2004); degradation of the learners’ memory capabilities (boman etal., 2005). furthermore, apart from being a barrier to the learning process, noise can also be perceived as a nuisance by children. young children exposed to high levels of noise in their environments report high levels of annoyance with the specific sounds that they are exposed to (cohen, evans, krantz, stokols & kelly, 1981). that is, young children are sensitive to noises in their environment and can contact: dr lebogang ramma division of communication sciences and disorders university of cape town groote schuur hospital, f45 old main building observa­ tory, 7925 cape town, south africa email: lebogang.ramma@uct.ac.za the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 35 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) mailto:lebogang.ramma@uct.ac.za discriminate between those noise sources that annoy them and the ones that do not annoy them (dockrell & shield, 2004; manlove, frank & vernon-feagans, 2001). classrooms with poor acoustic environments impact negatively on the educational achievement and performance of all learners in the class (knecht, nelson & whitelaw, 2002), however there are certain categories of children who are at a higher risk for the negative effects of too much background noise in the classroom. these include young children at various stages of language devel­ opment, learners with home languages different from the lan­ guage of learning, as well as those with hearing loss (crandell & smaldino, 2000). a study by broom (2004) revealed that most south african primary school classrooms have learners who are learning in a language that is not necessarily their home lan­ guage. this means that most south african primary school learn­ ers belong to a category of learners who can be considered to be at a higher risk of the negative effects of poor classroom acous­ tics. there has not yet been a detailed national study looking at the status of the acoustic environment’of classrooms in south african schools. a pilot study conducted in 2006 involving measurement of background noise levels in 15 classrooms from 15 primary schools in the johannesburg metropolitan area showed that the majority of these classrooms had background noise levels that exceeded both the south african national standards (sans) and table 1: background noise levels and reverberation times (rt) measured in 15 johannesburg primary schools lebogang ramma world health organization’s (who) standards of 35-40 dba and 0.6 seconds for maximum background noise levels and maxi­ mum reverberation times respectively, recommended for an un­ occupied classroom (ramma, 2007). typical strategies for addressing poor classroom acoustics the practice in developed countries such as the united states of america for dealing with poor acoustic environments in a class­ room involve the use of services of professionals such as acousti­ cal consultants and educational audiologists. typical interven­ tions include physical modification to the classroom space to enhance its acoustic characteristics, and / or installation of hear­ ing-assistive technology to overcome the problem of noise in the classroom (american speech-language hearing association (asha), 2005). such intervention measures may not be feasible in a south african context due to the scarcity of skilled personnel such as acoustical consultants and audiologists required to pro­ vide these services, and budget constraints to pay for modifica­ tions and assistive technology that may be required in these classrooms. however, one way to address the problem of poor classroom acoustics in south african primary schools could be to work with teachers and to empower them to intervene when their class­ rooms are not acoustically appropriate. when given the right strategies, teachers responsible for day-to-day management of the classroom environment can play a crucial role in managing background noise levels and amount of reverberation in their classrooms. examples of simple and yet effective strategies that can be shared with teachers include: closing doors or windows (especially those facing noise sources such as traf­ fic noise from busy streets), padding learners’ chairs with old tennis balls (de villiers, 2003) or even removing most of the reflective materials (e.g. mirrors) from the walls to lower the amount of reverberation. other strategies may involve plan­ ning of lessons in a way that takes into account noise levels during the course of the day, with subjects that require more reading being taught during less noisy times than other sub­ jects (boman et al., 2005). location schools unoccupied background noise level unoccupied rt60 value suburban s1 39.9 0.87 s2 40.52 0.91 s3 41.85 0.86 s4 42.81 0.79 s5 45.2 0.98 s6 45.54 0.59 township s1 40.5 0.78 s2 41.04 0.88 s3 45.08 0.81 s4 52.52 0.66 school close to a noise source s1 40.94 0.72 s2 43.06 0.71 s3 48.02 0.95 s4 51.81 0.72 s5 62.5 0.51 36 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) knowledge and attitudes of teachers regarding the impact of classroom acoustics on speech perception and learning according to asha (2005), the negative effect of classrooms with poor acoustic environments is not self-evident to teachers. unless this information is brought to their attention, teachers do not usually treat poor classroom acoustics as barriers to learning (asha, 2005). the purpose of this study therefore was to explore the knowledge and attitudes of school teachers in selected south african primary schools regarding the impact of too much back­ ground noise and reverberation in the class room on learners’ speech perception and learning. for the purposes of this study, the criteria for adequate knowl­ edge would be met if the respondents indicated that they had some type of input about the subject matter (i.e. impact of poor classroom acoustics on speech perception), and if their responses in the questionnaire showed that they were aware of the impact that too much background noise and reverberation in the class­ room (i.e. poor classroom acoustics) or poor quality of the teacher’s voice would have on speech perception and the learning process . that is, a declarative knowledge about these topics at a relational level of understanding when using the solo taxonomy (biggs & tang, 2007). according to biggs and tang (2007), declarative knowledge refers to “knowing about things, or knowing what,” while a relational level of understanding requires one to, among other things, ‘relate’ things or concepts (p.79). in this study, teachers were required to relate impact of noise to speech perception and learning. m e th o d aim to explore the knowledge and attitude of teachers in selected south african primary schools regarding the impact of poor class­ room acoustics on learners’ speech perception and learning. objectives the objectives of this study were therefore as follows: ' [ j o determine teachersj knowledge of classroom acoustics (background noise and reverberation) and its impact on speech perception and learning in class. [ j o determine teachers' knowledge about the level of teacher’s voice during classroom communication and its impact on speech perception and learning. > [ j o ascertain teachers’ opinions and attitudes regarding class­ room acoustics, the level of teachers voice and the impact of these factors on speech perception and learning. [ j o identify strategies used by teachers to enhance speech per­ ception and audibility in class. [ j o determine whether there is any association between teach­ ing experience or type of qualification and knowledge of classroom acoustics. research design to meet the aims of this study a descriptive cross-sectional sur­ vey research design (bowling, 2009) using quantitative data collec­ tion methods was used. participants to qualify as a participant in the research, subjects had to be currently practising in the teaching profession at primary school level. therefore, participants included primary school principals, deputy-principals and classroom teachers. school principals and deputy principals at all of the three participating schools were also included as participants in this study because they are also directly involved in day-to-day classroom teaching. sampling purposive sampling was used to select participating schools. schools were selected such that one school was from a quiet jo­ hannesburg suburb, the other school was from a major johannes­ burg township and the last school was located in a johannesburg inner-city area (close to most noisy areas). at each school, partici­ pants were recruited to participate in this study by way of conven­ ience sampling. all teachers that were present at each of the schools on the day of the survey were invited to participate in this study (and all those who were recruited agreed to participate). a total of 70 teachers (including principals and deputy principals) volunteered to participate in this study. fifty-four of the participants were female and sixteen were male. forty-four had more than 10 years’ teaching experience and twenty-six had less than 10 years teaching experience. twenty-three of the participants had a four year university degree in education or higher while the remaining forty-seven had a diploma in education or equivalent. all of the participants reported using english as the primary medium of in­ struction in their classes. data collection procedure permission was first requested from gauteng education depart­ ment to conduct the study. this project was also approved by the university of the witwatersrand human research ethics committee (non-medical) (protocol number h070205). once permission and ethical clearance were granted, participating schools and partici­ pants in this study were selected as described above. school princi­ pals were contacted by telephone to request their permission and consent to participate in this study. if they gave permission, an appointment was made at a time convenient for the principal and the entire staff to go and administer the survey questionnaire. at all three schools, the survey was conducted either during the staff lunch break or at the beginning of the weekly staff meeting to avoid disrupting teaching activities. each participant was given an information sheet explaining the aims of the study and a consent form to indicate their willingness (or lack of) to take part in the study. after the aim of the study was explained to participants and consent forms were signed, each participant was given the survey questionnaire to complete. the researcher was physically present to answer any questions that arose during the completion of the questionnaire. questionnaires were collected immediately upon completion, and participants were thanked for their participation. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 37 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) data collection tool a specially constructed questionnaire developed by the re­ searcher was used to gather information for this study. the ques­ tionnaire was anonymous and self-administered. it comprised mostly close-ended questions, and contained the following four sections (see appendix): [] section a: biographical information; g section b: specific knowledge on classroom acoustics; [] section c: opinion and attitude of participants regard­ ing classroom acoustics and its impact on classroom learning; and q section d: strategies used to enhance audibility and speech perception in the classroom. pilotstudy the questionnaire was piloted with 10 participants with similar characteristics to the intended study sample to establish reliabil­ ity and content validity (maxwell & satake, 2006) as well as to obtain representative and unbiased feedback about the ques­ tionnaire. the results of pilot testing revealed that teachers were not familiar with most of the technical terms or phrases that are commonly used when discussing room acoustics. the most prob­ lematic words or phrases were ‘reverberation’, ‘speech percep­ tion’ and ‘classroom acoustics.’ these words (and or phrases) were therefore replaced with more familiar words: reverberation was replaced with ‘too many reflective surfaces in the class­ room’; speech perception was replaced with ‘hear and under­ stand speech’ and classroom acoustics was broken into ‘background noise’ and ‘too many reflective surfaces.’ data analysis the results of the questionnaire were analysed using a statisti­ cal software package (statistica-9) to establish patterns of re­ sponses between different variables in this study. the statistical test chosen for this purpose was the pearson chi-square (x2). this test is commonly used to compare the observed results with results that are expected based on a certain assumption or ac­ cording to specific hypotheses (maxwell & satake, 2006). for instance, in this study it was expected that newly qualified teach­ ers (e.g. <10 years experience) and teachers with at least a four year university degree in education will be more likely to report knowledge about classroom acoustics than teachers who hold diplomas in education or teachers who have been teaching longer (> 10 years experience). results this section will present the findings of the study as follows: (1) teachers self-reported knowledge of classroom acoustics; (2) attitude regarding classroom acoustics and the level of teacher’s voice in the class and the impact of these factors on speech lebogang ramma perception in class; (3) strategies used by teachers to enhance speech perception in class, and (4) association between teach­ ing experience, type of qualification and knowledge of classroom acoustics. seventy teachers from three primary schools completed the survey questionnaire for this study. however, eleven of the sev­ enty questionnaires (16%) had to be discarded because the par­ ticipants left an entire section of the questionnaire blank. only fifty-nine (84%) questionnaires were used in the analysis, there­ fore the findings reported in the following sections will be based only on responses from these questionnaires (n=59). 1) knowledge regarding classroom acoustics twenty-one (36%) of the participants reported that their classes were next to a noise source. traffic was the most fre­ quently cited source of noise, followed by noise from other learn­ ers in the playground. participants were then asked whether they had any formal input on classroom acoustics while they were training to become teachers and / or while working as teachers. twenty three (39%) of the participants reported that they had some type of input on classroom acoustics during their training as teachers, and eight (14%) said they had received some input about classroom noise via in-service training workshops. participants were then asked to rate their knowledge regarding the following three factors: too much background noise, too much reverberation in the classroom and level of teacher’s voice and furthermore, the impact of these three factors on speech perception (hearing and understanding speech) and learning in the classroom. a five-point likert scale was used with the follow­ ing categories: limited, satisfactory, average, good and very good. most teachers (fifty-one; 86%) rated themselves as having aver­ age to very good knowledge about the level of the teacher’s voice and its impact on learning in the classroom (see figure 1). forty-three (75%) of the respondents felt they had average or good knowledge about background noise and its impact on teachers' self-rating of their own knowledge 0 0 l im ite d s a tis fa c to ry a v e ra g e g o o d v e r y g o o d rating o f own knowledge figure 1: teachers' self-rating of their own knowledge regarding classroom acoustics 38 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) knowledge and attitudes of teachers regarding the impact of classroom acoustics on speech perception and learning classroom learning. none of the respondents reported very good knowledge of background noise and its impact on learning. thirty-six teachers (62%) rated their knowledge of the impact of having too many reflective surfaces in the classroom on learning as average or good. none of the respondents rated their knowl­ edge here as being very good. almost all of the respondents (with the exception of four) felt that teachers need some input on classroom acoustics and other factors that influence or affect speech perception in the classroom. seventeen (31%) of those who responded felt that this input should be given during teacher training, while sixteen (29%) felt that this should be given both during teacher training and as part of in-service train­ ing. 2) attitude regarding classroom acoustics and teacher’s voice and its impact on speech perception and learning most of the respondents in the survey (thirty-eight; 64%) were of the opinion that the teacher can play an important role in con­ trolling factors that can impact on speech perception and learn­ ing in the classroom (e.g. background noise). forty-four (75%) of the teachers who completed the questionnaire reported that they were satisfied with their classroom environment in terms of facili­ tating adequate speech perception by learners in the classroom. an overwhelming majority (87%) of the respondents agreed with the statement that if learners’ speech perception is compro­ mised due to poor acoustics in the classroom then overall aca­ demic achievement will be negatively impacted. however, when respondents were asked to rate the impact that too much back­ ground noise, too much reverberation in the classroom, and low level of the teacher’s voice may have on speech understanding l and learning in the classroom, most were of the opinion that / i these factors will only have an average (even little or no) impact on classroom learning (figure 2). j impact of classroom acoustics/teacher's voice on classroom learning 35 n o im p a ct little im pact a w r a g e im pa ct m ore im p a ct m ost im pact rating of impact figure 2: teachers’ views of impact of background noise, rever­ beration & level of the teacher’s voice on speech perception in the classroom. 3) strategies used b y teachers to enhance speech perception and understanding in class twenty-eight (47%) of the respondents reported that they often needed to raise their voices when talking to the learners during normal teaching activities. only 19 (32%) reported that they con­ sidered the acoustic environment of their classroom when plan­ ning or preparing for their daily lessons. the most common strategies used by respondents in this survey to deal with noisy classrooms were; "to speak louder, more clearly and more slowly". some respondents reported varying their voices according to the noise conditions, while some addressed a noisy classroom environ­ ment by organising the learners’ seating arrangement in the class­ room to favour learners with limited english language communica­ tion skills. one respondent reported playing classical music while students did written work as a strategy to help them concentrate better. 4) association between teaching experience, type of qualification and knowledge of classroom acoustics tables la and l b show the results of a pearson chi-square analy­ sis regarding an association between the following variables: 1 -teaching experience and knowledge of classroom acoustics (table 2a). table 2a: association between teaching experience and knowledge of classroom acoustics knows about classroom acoustics row totals experience (no. of yrs teaching) yes no <10 9 11 20 >10 27 12 39 column totals 36 23 59 pearson chi-square x 2 = 2.150096, df= 1, p= 0.14256 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 39 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 2 -type of qualification and knowledge of classroom acoustics (table 2b). table 2b: association between type of qualification and knowl­ edge of classroom acoustics lebogang ramma knowsledge about class­ room acoustics row totals type of qualifica­ tion yes no diploma 17 22 39 4 year degree or higher 7 13 20 column totals 24 35 59 pearson chi square x 2 = 0.9111253, df= 1, p= 0.33982 the results of this study showed that there was no association between teaching experience and the probability of being taught about classroom acoustics while training or while working (p = 0.05). there was also no association between the type of qualifi­ cation and reporting being taught about classroom acoustics while training or after qualifying as a teacher (p = 0.05). discussion school classrooms are the environments in which most of the learning activities at school occur. the quality of what the learn­ ers are expected to learn is therefore partially dependent on the environment in which they learn (i.e. the classroom environment). it is therefore important that the classroom environment should be free of extraneous distractions (e.g. too much background noise) that can interfere with the learning process. despite the fact that the suitability of learning spaces is planned by architec­ tural designers as early as the design of the school or classroom building (guckelberger, 2003), teachers are ultimately the people with more control of the daily management of that space. this was a view that was also held by the majority of the participants (64%) in this study it was encouraging to see that some of the participants had received some input on classroom acoustics either during train­ ing (36%) or after their training as teachers (12%). however, over­ all the results of this study showed that the majority of partici­ pants did not have adequate knowledge about the topic of class­ room acoustics and the impact that a classroom with a poor acoustic environment could have on the learning process. these findings are consistent with those of other studies, such as the one by dockrell et al. (2004), in which it was found that despite the fact that noise is a serious problem in urban schools in the united kingdom, teachers show little awareness of the impor­ tance of noise levels in their classrooms and of the need to moni­ tor noise for particular tasks and teaching contexts. participants in this study generally rated their knowledge of classroom acoustics and the importance of voice in classroom learning as high (average or better). however, it was evident from responses in later parts of the questionnaire that despite this high self-rated knowledge, most participants did not have ade­ quate knowledge of the impact that a sub-optimal classroom acoustic environment may have on the learners’ speech percep­ tion in class. for instance, almost half (44%) of the participants were of the opinion that excessive background noise in the class will only have an average impact on speech understanding in the classroom while a third (33%) of the respondents were of the opinion that deterioration in the teacher’s voice will have an aver­ age impact on speech understanding. one explanation for this observed high self-rating could be the fact that these topics, es­ pecially background noise and teacher’s voice are generally intui­ tive phrases to an average teacher. therefore, teachers may be familiar with the phrase or words because they are part of every­ day language in a school setting. however, that does not neces­ sarily mean that they are fully aware of the impact that these factors may have on speech perception and consequently learn­ ing in the classroom. participants also tended to assume that background noise had a greater negative impact on speech perception and learning than the teacher’s voice .this is despite the fact that the negative effect that background noise has in a classroom situation is due to the fact that it covers up part of the teacher’s voice and hence degrades the quality of the message communicated to the learn­ ers (crandell & smaldino, 1994). the teacher’s voice is therefore the most important variable in a classroom situation. any situa­ tion or event in the classroom that compromises the teacher’s voice is bound to have the most impact in the learning process. it was therefore expected that participants would rate the impact of the teacher’s voice on the learning process higher than any other factors or variable. surprisingly, a third (33%) of the teachers who participated in this study were of the opinion that the level of the teacher’s voice is less important than too much background in terms of impact on speech perception and learning in the class­ room. most respondents (75%) reported that they were satisfied, with / the suitability of their classroom environment as "a learning envi­ ronment. this is consistent with the findings of another study (manlove et al., 2001), in which teachers tended to be more ac-/ commodating of noisy classrooms and often dismissed excessive background interference as the “price of doing business.” this accommodating attitude is likely to be prevalent if the interfering 40 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) knowledge and attitudes of teachers regarding the impact of classroom acoustics on speech perception and learning noise is from learners on the school property (manlove et al., 2001) as opposed to noise sources external to the school envi­ ronment (e.g. traffic noise), even though meaningless, irrelevant speech (noise from an adjacent class or other learners in the playground) has been shown to have the greater effect on inter­ fering with speech perception than other types of noise (boman, enmarker & hygge, 2005). almost half of the respondents said that they often need to raise their voices above their normal talking levels when commu­ nicating with learners. this need to raise their voices could be an indication of excessive background noise levels in the classroom. doyle & dye (2003) advised that the best approach to dealing with interfering background noise in the classroom is to be aware of sources of noise (both extraneous and internal) and to start planning teaching activities with this in mind rather than simply talking louder. in the present study only 32% of respon­ dents said that they considered the acoustic environment of their classroom when planning their lessons. none mentioned any of the common strategies that involve physical modification of the classroom (e.g. carpeting the classroom floors, cushioning the legs of the learners’ chairs) as a strategy for enhancing the acoustics. implications of the study and future research this study showed that the majority of the teachers who took part in this study did not have adequate knowledge about poor classroom acoustics. teachers therefore need to be given more information on this and also to be trained to recognise the need for an intervention when working in classroom environments that are acoustically inappropriate. teachers also need to be trained on simple and yet effective)solutions to use when the classroom environment is not conducive to learning due to poor classroom /a co u s tic s . this should ide'ally be addressed in the curriculum during teacher training, asjwell as in refresher courses for later in-service training for teachers. the results of this study showed that there was no association between teaching experience or the type of qualification that the teacher holds and the likelihood of having received some | input on classroom acoustics. ■'this means that refresher courses should target all teachers who are currently working regardless of teaching experiences or the type of qualification they hold. while this study attempted to investigate key aspects of class­ room acoustics (e.g. background noise and reverberation), it was not possible to provide an in-depth investigation of this subject matter due to time and resource constraints. future studies should aim to explore the feasibility of training current teachers to manage the acoustic environment of the classroom to opti­ mise its use as a space for learning. limitations of the study since information for this study was obtained by using a ques­ tionnaire with predominantly close-ended questions, respon­ dents were limited on what they could say because they were not given the opportunity to elaborate on their responses. the ques­ tionnaire also contained many terms that were not necessarily familiar to the teachers. while an attempt was made to minimise the number of unfamiliar words during the piloting of the ques­ tionnaire, as well as during a briefing before completing it, some of the respondents may have found the questionnaire to be time consuming since they were dealing with a subject not necessarily familiar to them. lastly, this survey was conducted with teachers from only a three schools using non-random sampling and par­ ticipant selection methods. although an attempt was made to ensure that the schools selected were heterogeneous and repre­ sented diverse geographical areas, it is not possible to general­ ize the findings of this study to all teachers or schools in the johannesburg area or to other parts of the country. conclusion the results of this study showed that teachers who partici­ pated in this study did not have adequate knowledge about classroom acoustics. knowledge was especially lacking when it came to indicating how classroom environments with poor acoustics are likely to affect speech perception, and conse­ quently the learning process. given the linguistic diversity of most classrooms in south african primary schools, it means that most learners who have not mastered the language of learning are likely to be disadvantaged when the classroom environment is not acoustically suitable. this means that teachers need to be given the necessary input to manage the acoustic environment of their classrooms. different stakeholders, such as teachers, acoustics experts and educational audiologists should come together to address this issue. management of the acoustic envi­ ronment of the classroom should ideally also be addressed dur­ ing teacher training. for teachers who are currently working but do not have adequate knowledge about classroom acoustics and its impact on the learning process, refresher courses could be offered to equip them with strategies to better manage the acoustic environments of their classrooms. acknowledgement i thank the gauteng provincial department of education for granting me the permission to conduct this study. references asha (2005). guidelines for addressing acoustics in educa­ tional settings [guidelines]. available from www.asha.org/ policy. anderson, k. (2004). the problem of classroom acoustics: the typical classroom soundscape is a barrier to learning. semin hear, 25, 117-128. biggs, j.b. & tang, c. (2007). teaching for quality learning at university, (pp. 72-80). berkshire, uk: open university press. boman, e., enmarker, i. & hygge, s. (2005). strength of noise the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 41 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) http://www.asha.org/ effects on memory as a function of noise source and age. noise and health 7 (27): 11-26. bowling, a. (2009). research methods in health: investigating health and health services (pp. 185-235). new york, usa: open university press. broom, y. (2004). reading english in multilingual south african primary schools. international journal of bilingual education & bilingualism 7(6): 506-528. cohen, s., evans, g.w., krantz, d.s., stokols, d., & kelly, s. (1981). aircraft noise and children: longitudinal and cross sectional evidence on adaptation to noise and the effectiveness of noise abatement. j pers soc/psychol 40(2): 331-345. crandell, c.c. & smaldino, j j . (2000). room acoustics for listen­ ers with normal hearing and hearing impairment. in: m. valente, h. hosford-dunn & r. roeser (eds). audiology treat­ ment. (pp. 601-637) new york: thiem e,. crandell, c. & smaldino, j. (1994). the importance of room acoustics. in: r. tyler & d. schum (eds.). assistive listening device for the hearing impaired, (pp. 142-164) baltimore, md: williams & w ilkins.. de villiers, r. (2003). factors that influence the reception of spo­ ken language, in sound aspirations : a specialized and informa­ tive program for educators who work with learners with hearing loss in mainstream schools. tygerberg, south africa. carel du toit centre. unpublished. dockrell, j.e., shield, b.m. & rigby, k (2004). acoustic guidelines and teacher strategies for optimising learning conditions in classrooms for children with hearing problems. in: d.a. fabry, c. deconde & johnson, z. (eds), access: achieving clear com­ munication employing sound solutions-3003. proceedings of the first international fm conference, (pp. 217-227) cambrian printers ltd. great britain.. dockrell, j.e. & shield, b. (2004). children’s perceptions of their acoustic environment at school and at home. j acoustic soc am 115(6): 2964-2973. doyle, m. & dye, l., (2003). mainstreaming the student who is deaf and hard of hearing. a guide for professionals, teachers and parents. san diego: director cchat. guckelberger, d. (2003). acoustics in the classroom. engineers newsletter, 3 2 :1-11. hicks, c.b. & tharpe, a.m (2002). listening effort and fatigue in school-age children with and without hearing loss. journal o f speech, language, and hearing research, 45(3): 573-584. knecht, h.a., nelson, p. b. & whitelaw, g. m. (2002). background noise levels and reverberation times in unoccupied class­ rooms: predictions and measurements. american journal of audiology, 11: 65-71. lundquist, p., holmberg, k. & landstrom, u. (2000). annoyance and effects on work from environmental noise at school. noise and health, 28: 39-46. mackenzie, d. (2000). noise sources and levels in uk schools. international symposium on noise control and acoustics for educational buildings. proceedings, turkish acoustical society, istanbul, may 2000: 97-106. manlove, e.e., frank, t. & vernon-feagans (2001). why should we care about noise in classrooms and child care settings? child & youth care forum, 30(1): 55-64. lebogang ramma massaro, d.w. (2001). speech perception. in n.m smelser & p.b. baltes (eds), international encyclopedia of social and behavioural sciences (pp. 14870-14875). amsterdam, the netherlands: elsevier. maxwell, l. & evans, g. (2000). the effects of noise on pre­ school childrens’ pre-reading skills. journal of environmental psychology, 20: 91-97. maxwell, l.d.& satake, e. (2006). research and statistical meth­ ods in communication sciences and disorders (pp. 279-313). boston, usa: thomson/delmar learning. ramma, l. (2007). rethinking our classrooms: assessment of background noise levels and reverberation in schools. educa­ tion as change, 11(2): 115-130. shield, b. & dockrell, j.e. (2004). the effects of noise on chil­ dren at school: a review. building acoustics 10: 97-116. shield, b.m., dockrell, j.e., asker, r., & tachmatzidis, i. (2002). effects of noise on attainments and cognitive performance of primary school children. report for department of health, march 2002. 42 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) knowledge and attitudes of teachers regarding the impact of classroom acoustics on speech perception and learning appendix speech perception survey questionnaire directions for completing the questionnaire: 1. please use ink pen 2. please mark with an “x” to indicate your selections a. demographic information 1. gender i i male i ifemale 2. number o f years teaching □ 0 1 q 2 5 0 6 1 0 o > 1 0 3. highest education qualification □ d i p . prim. ed. d b . prim ed. d b a ed. d m a . ed. d o th erj____________ 4. where did you qualify? specify training institution: ________________________ 5. current post at the school i iclassroom teacher | |deputy principal i iprincipal 6. grade currently teaching (please select one) □ o m i \ j 2 d 3 d 4 d 5 d 6 d 7 d 8 h |9 d l o i 7. grades taught in the past 5 years (select a maximum of 3) □ o m i \ b 2 d 3 d 4 d 5 d 6 d 7 d 8 d 9 d l o i l 8. current number o f learners in your class □ < 2 0 □ 2 0 2 5 0 2 5 3 0 d 3 0 -3 5 d 3 5 -4 0 d 4 0 -4 5 d > 4 5 9. primary language used as a medium o f instruction in class (please select 1) i lenglish i lisizulu i isesotho ^ a frik a a n s i lisixhosa i i o th e r______________ 10. number o f learners in your classroom who speak english as their first/home language (estim ate)________________ 11. is your school/classroom located close to a noise source (e.g. freeway, busy street etc.)? □ y e s d no 12. if yes above, please specify the nature of noise source: ___________________ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 43 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) lebogang ramma 13.does your classroom have the following: a. double wall construction b. acoustical ceiling tile in hallways c. fully carpeted floor d. brick wall e. posters on the walls f. curtains/blinds in all windows g. concrete floors h. ceramic tiles on the floor i. ceramic tiles on the walls j. vinyl tiles on the floor yes no not sure yes no b. knowledge on speech perception and classroom acoustics 1. w hile you were training as a teacher, did your training curriculum include: a. background noise and its impact on learning b. reflective surface in class and their impact on learning c. teaching in a multi-language context d. optimizing speech understanding b y pupils in the classroom e. vocal hygiene and voice projection 2. since you started working as a teacher, have you had additional continuing education training on: yes no a. background noise and its impact on learning b. reflective surfaces in class and their impact on learning c. teaching in a multi-language context d. optimizing speech understanding by pupils in the classroom e. vocal hygiene and voice projection 3. are you aware o f any legislation, national education department policy or national standard that deals with any o f the following? yes no a. learner to teacher ratio in the classroom □ □ if yes, please specify: b. maximum background noise in the classroom if yes, please specify: □ □ c. maximum reverberation time in the classroom if yes, please specify: □ □ d. l anguage o f instruction in the classroom i f y es, p lea se sp ecify: □ □ 44 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) h ow would you rate your knowledge o f the follow ing factors and their impact on speech understanding and learning in the classroom: a. background noise ^ l im it e d □ s a tisfa c to r y | [average | [good | |very good b. reverberant classroom □ l im ite d □ s a tisfa c to r y | [average | [good | [very good c. signal to noise ratio □ l im ite d □ s a tisfa c to r y | [average | [good | |very good d. listening distance □ l im ite d l~~lsatisfactory □ [a v era g e i igood i ivery good e . hearing ability o f individual learners □ l im ite d □ s a tisfa c to r y | [average | [good | [very good f. linguistic experience o f the learners □ l im ite d ^ s a tis fa c to r y □ average □ g o o d | [very good g. leve 1 o f te acher ’ s voice □ l im ite d □ s a tisfa c to r y | [average | [good | |very good h. too many reflective surfaces in the classroom □ l im ite d □ s a tisfa cto ry | [average | [good | [very good do you ihink teachers need specific training on the above m entioned factors that influence/affect speech perception in class? yes no knowledge and attitudes of teachers regarding the impact of classroom acoustics on speech perception and learning □ □ i f you answered y e s in 6 above, when should this training occur? y e s n o a. during teacher training in teacher training colleges b. in service training as continuing education c. both (during training and as an in-service training) □ □ □ □ □ □ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 45 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) lebogang ramma c. opinion regarding classroom acoustics and its im pact on speech understanding and learning 1. are you satisfied with your classroom environment as far as facilitating learners’ ability to hear and understand speech? yes no □ □ 2. i f no, please explain (use back page i f you need more space) 3. in your opinion who has the greatest control on the afore mentioned factors that influence hearing and speech understanding in the classroom? (please do not select more than 2 choices) i i classroom/school designers and planners i i school administrators (e.g. principal) i i classroom teacher i i national department o f education 4. rate the follow ing factors on how much impact they w ill have on speech understanding and learning in the class room? a. level o f the teacher’s noise i |n o impact i i little impact □ average impact i i more impact i |most impact b.too many reflective surfaces in the classroom i ino impact i ilittle impact i i average impact i |more impact | |most impact c. background in the classroom i ino impact i ilittle impact □ a v e r a g e impact i |more impact | |most impact d.hearing ability o f the pupils in classroom i |no impact i ilittle impact i i average impact i |more impact | |most impact e. reverberation in the classroom □ n o impact i ilittle impact q average impact i i more impact | |most impact f. linguistic experience o f the learners □ n o impact □ l it t le impact i i average impact i |more impact | |most impact g. listening distance □ n o impact □ l it t le impact i laverage impact □ m o r e impact □ m o s t impact 46 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) knowledge and attitudes of teachers regarding the impact of classroom acoustics on speech perception and learning 5. in your opinion, poor hearing and speech understanding in class w ill lead to: a gree d isagree n ot sure a. poor reading/spelling skills □ □ □ b. poor mathematics and science skills n □ □ c. behavior problems n □ □ d. attention and concentration problems □ □ □ e. poor academic achievement □ □ □ 6. in your opinion, what acoustic features in your specific classroom impact negatively on speech understanding? please list as many as you can: d.strategies to enhance hearing and understanding speech as well as audibility in the classroom 1. do you need to raise your voice when talking to your students during normal teaching activities? □ yes d n o 2. do you teach with: y e s n o a. d oor open n n b. w indows open □ □ c. fan on ' □ □ 3. do you consider the acoustic environment o f your classroom when you plan and prepare for your lesson? □ yes d n o 4. do you have learners with special listening needs (e.g. hearing impairment etc.) in your classroom? □ yes d n o 5. list some o f the things that you do or have done to improve speech understanding in class. list: 6. i f you have concerns about the status o f your classroom acoustics, what should you do to address that? list: 7. “ the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 47 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) journal of the south african logopedic society september e d i t o r i a l in this issue an attempt has been made to cover some of the so-called "physical" types of speech defect. greater emphasis is being placed on a wholistic approach to these problems, where the individual is treated as a whole and all his needs, not merely his handicap, are taken into account. it is generally considered that, where doctors and medical auxiliaries work together as a team, a fuller picture of the individual's needs is obtained, and a more comprehensive therapy programme can be planned. drs. jacobson and dreyer in their article on cleft palate have stressed the need for teamwork in this field, while mrs. vorwerg has shown how it is being employed in cenles for the cerebral palsied throughout the world. mrs. bauman, too, shows the need for considering the patient's psychological reactions to his handicap, as well as the actual teaching of speech and voice to the laryngectomee. this edition therefore, indicates the need for the speech therapist to relate her work to that of other workers in the field of rehabilitation. protefl holdings limited hearing aid division 201 philadelphia corner — jeppe & von weilligh streets p.o. box 7793 johannesburg phone 22-0511 distributors and stockists of: peters clinical and diagnostic audiometers multitone telesonic induction loop system (for complete freedom of movement for all pupils) multitone individual auditory trainers sonotone — multitone — oticon hearing aids conventional and spectacle — air and bone conduction ethically prescribed and fitted. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d c society projective interpretation of intelligence tests y. a. lejeune, ph.d. psychologist, johannesburg child guidance clinic. intelligence test results given in the form of iq· only a r e often very misleading, particularly where the subject is a deviate one, a s is the c a s e with most children seen at child guidance, or speech clinics. in all c a s e s a qualitative report is n e c e s s a r y , dealing with the subject's attitudes to ihe test situation and to the examiner, in order to determine whether the i.q. is a valid one or n o t ; a n d in addition a good test report must include some assessment of the subject's personality. this is particularly important where it is not possible to give a battery of personality tests a s an aid to diagnosis, a n d for this reason projective interpretation of intelligence test responses is useful, b e c a u s e it can throw light on aspects of personality which may not b e revealed in other diagnostic interviews. a standard intelligence test differs from a true projective test mainly in the d e g r e e of structuring of the stimuli presented to the subject. the questions a r e framed in such a w a y that there is—or should b e — v e r y little ambiguity, a n d in theory the questions should h a v e exactly the s a m e meaning for all subjects. in practice, however, there is a wide variation in the amount of ambiguity present in the test stimuli, a n d in many sub-test items there is ample opportunity for the subject to interpret the meaning of the stimulus in terms of his own personality drives. as a general rule, the more neurotic the subject, the more likely he is to produce these projective responses. some e x a m p l e s of responses to the s.a. individual s c a l e of intelligence m a y illustrate this point. leven a fairly structured test, " a r e you a little1 girl/or a little b o y ? " m a y produce a response which is emotionally determined. a response of "no, i'm b i g " is often related to feeling of insecurity, a n d frequently a girl m a y insist that she is a boy, even though she knows quite well that she is not. less often a boy says that he is a girl, b e c a u s e of a fear of his own masculinity. th ere is another group of items in the individual s c a l e , where the 1 questions a r e less structured, but which produce deviate responses in the less adjusted c a s e s . i would include in this category the comprehension tests, which usually produce a standard success or failure, but which m a y also reveal attitudes of dependence. or withdrawal. a very dependent child may preface his replies with "ask my mommy," a n d in one c a s e of a n overprotected child with hypochondriacal symptoms, the answer to " w h a t should you do b e fore beginning something difficult?" w a s " r e s t . " timid, over-anxious children, or ihose with strong guilt feelings, tend to produce response which stress the d a n g e r s of the outside world, a n d they will do this even when the test questions a r e moderately structured, e.g., "a snaxe, a cow a n d a bird a r e alike b e c a u s e they c a n all hurt you;" or, in the sentence-making test, "a boy swam in the river a n d cut himself on a stone a n d was drowned," instead of the more usual "a boy threw a stone in the river." distortions resulting from timidity may also app e a r in the memories of the reading p a s s a g e , if the subject " r e c a l l s " death or injury. sibling rivalry or interest in the birth of a new 'baby m a y also b e shown in these tests, e.g., in one c a s e of this type the s u b j e c t ' s responses to the differences test were in terms of origin—' a stone comes from the ground, and a n e g g from a hen, milk comes from a cow and water from a t a p " ; a n d this w a s followed b y what a p p e a r e d to be a birth-interest response on the similarities tesi—"a dog a n d a horse a r e the s a m e b e c a u s e they've both got big tummies." in addition to these test items, there a r e three sub-items on the individual s c a l e where the stimuli a r e so little structured that they might almost be thought of a s projective items. the first of these is the picture interpretation test occurring at y e a r 1 3 . the pictures themselves a r e not a s a mb ig uous a s those of a thematic apperception test, a n d the emotions depicted a r e fairly c l e a r ; but the c a u s e of the emotions is not clear, a n d for this reason the interpretations given m a y b e very revealing about the subject's anxieties or wishes. references to food deprivation, or quarrelling between the parents—"the father's left them, a n d they have nobody to look after t h e m " ^ do not necessarily reflect the real home situation, but these responses do indicate the n a t u r e of the child's phantasies and sources' of anxiety. another test of this type is the word a s s o ciation test. for the most part ihe normal child will give words which a r e inspired b y what he s e e s around him—"pencil, pen, block, e t c . " — b u t the more disturbed subject is likely to produce responses which a r e a s s o c i a t e d r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 j o u r n a l o f t h e south a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society may with p h a n t a s y . o n e girl sex-deliquent produced 35 b o y s ' n a m e s within one minute, an achievement which most people would find impossible to emulate. other subjects of the s a m e type m a y produce words associated with exhibitionistic behaviour—e.g., referring to dress, parties, ornaments, etc. another frequent type is the response met with a m o n g regressed or immature subjects, the food response, where a large number of the words a r e related to food or e a t i n g ; a n d a m o n g the more a g g r e s s i v e children the words may refer to fighting—weapons, battles, etc. the third test of this type is perhaps the most revealing of all, a n d h a s b e e n selected for special study. this is t h e . ball-and-field test, which from our point of view has two a d v a n t a g e s over all the other sub-tests in the scale. in the first p l a c e it a p p e a r s at two levels on the s.a. individual s c a l e — a t y e a r 9 a n d y e a r 12, so that nearly all primary school children, a n d a large number of high school children a r e likely to do the test. and in the second place, it is' the least structured of all the test items, a n d therefore produces more emotionally conditioned responses than any of the other items. subjects and procedure in the present s t u d y : ι the s u b j e c t s for this study were 200 children seen a t the j o h a n n e s b u r g child g u i d a n c e clinic, selected on the b a s i s of the most recent c a s e s who h a d done the ball-and-field test a s part of [the routine individual s c a l e examination. t?he a g e r a n g e w a s 5 y e a r s 7 months to 15 years, with a median a g e of 9 y e a r s 4 months.' mental a g e r a n g e w a s 6 years 3 months j to 15 years 8 months, median 9 y e a r s 2 months, a n d the i.q. r a n g e w a s 64 to 148, median 96. in all c a s e s the ball-and-field test w a s given a s part of the s.a. individual s c a l e , with the standard instructions, but in addition the reaction time a n d total time taken for the test were recorded, a s it w a s felt that deviations from the norm in time scores might indicate the· presence of anxiety. any comments on the'test b y the subject, or questions a b o u t it w e r e also noted. the records of the group were then divided into p a s s e s or failures, using the 9-year level scoring a s a b a s i s for a pass, and then these two groups were further sub-divided on the b a s i s of similarity of patterns. the psychiatric diagnosis for e a c h c a s e w a s then compared with others in the s a m e sub-group, in order to d e t e r m i n e d whether the ball-and-field patterns could b e correlated with patterns of personality organisation. results. a. p a s s e s : group 1—normal passes, with normal reaction time (below 7 " ) . ( s e e diagrams 1-3). this group consisted of 61 cases, of whom 44 were diagnosed a s "well adjusted." group 2—normal p a s s e s (similar to group 1) but with excessively long reaction t i m e s — over 7". there were 13 c a s e s in this group, all of whom were diagnosed a s responding to "excessive frustration" ; i.e., although not well-adjusted, these children were not typical c a s e s of maladjusted personalities. group 3—vertical lines (diagram 4). the 5 c a s e s in this group were all diagnosed a s "having marked feelings of deprivation, demanding indulgence, or narcissistic." diagram 5, a combination of " n o r m a l " hcrizonal lines a n d vertical ones (diagram 5) w a s produced b y a child referred to the clinic for stealing, a n d w a s diagnosed a s "feeling deprived, b e c a u s e of the mother's self-indulgence" ; but the stealing stopped after one interview with the child, and two with the mother, so that it was apparent that the maladjustment w a s not very deep-rooted. group 4—excessively careful filling of the field (diagram 6). *a11 5 c a s e s in this group were diagnosed a s obsessional, a n d 4 of them were enuretic. three were having difficulty at school b e c a u s e of "slow work." group 5—success followed b y flight from the field (diagram 7). the 3 c a s e s in this group were described a s "over-conscientious," but 2 of them showed, conversion symptoms, a n d one was referred for school difficulty. group 6—the " p h a l l i c " response (diagram 8). the naming of this pattern w a s a psychologist's hunch, but w a s borne out b y the problems shown b y the 7 c a s e s in this group. five of them were referred for sex difficulties, and the other 2 were enuretics, whose enuresis w a s related to over-stimulation of sex interests. group 7—confused p a s s e s (diagram 9). of the 19 c a s e s in this group, 16 were described in the psychiatric report a s "timid" or " a p prehensive", 14 of them were referred for school difficulty, although only 4 of them h a v e i.q.'s below 90. they a r e apparently children whose anxiety leads to confusion in thinking. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) may j o u r n a l o f t h e south a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society ! 1 group 8—normal pattern, long reaction time, a c c o m p a n i e d b y much questioning of the examiner.! these two c a s e s were both referred for nosed a s school difficulties, a n d were diagb e i n g dependent a n d submissive. i b. failures : ι in assessing the importance of these patterns, the questions should b e a s k e d : " w h a t causes failure on this test? in some c a s e s it may b e sheer intellectual i n a d e q u a c y , but in the c a s e s studied, 6 2 % of the children had mental a g e s over 9 years, a n d should h a v e passed the test at the 9 y e a r level, while a further 1 4 % h a d mental a g e s over 8 y e a r s 6 months, a n d might possibly h a v e p a s s e d it, as they p a s s e d other tests at the 9 y e a r level. in this group, therefore, it is possible that failure w a s due to emotional factors, related to the g e n e r a l neurotic pattern of failure on nonverbal tests, or to anxiety a s a result of the lack of structuring in the stimulus. the type of failure throws more light on this emotional factor. in none of the failure groups a r e there more than 1 0 % of the c a s e s a n d it is therefore not possible to talk a b o u t a "normal failure." group 9—single straight line (diagram 10). of the 20 c a s e s in this group, 11 were described a s being " n e g a t i v e " or. "resentful to the mother," a n d 9 were diagnosed a s "timid, dependent or overprotected." it would a p p e a r that this is the pattern of least effort, a n d that this m a y arise either from unwillingness to follow the test instructions, or from d e p e n d e n c e a n d helplessness. group 10—a circle with or without a line to the centre (diagram 11 a n d 12). 13 of the 16 c a s e s in this group were described a s timid,, insecure or withdrawn, and 15 oi them had i.q.'s below 90. this a p p e a r s to b e the pattern of the timid, i n a d e q u a t e personality, with a low i.q. group 11—drawing the ball (diagram 13). of the 9 c a s e s in this group, 5 were referred for stealing, a n d the other 4, referred for a g g r e s s i v e behaviour, c a m e from materially deprived homes. all 9 h a v e i.q., i.e, all these c a s e s h a v e a n interest in material possessions, and all h a v e difficulty in dealing with problems requiring abstract thinking b e c a u s e of the low level of intelligence. group 12—following the outline (diagram 14). these 8 c a s e s were all referred for school r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) may ^ ttz ϊα the other c h d d . ^ t h ot ' u j n α a s c a s e s the parents v e . e c h i l d r e n ' s restrictive ^ a ' o i ^ q { demands ihis a p p t h e t e γγ.γ.ογ their own ^ t h e i r " parents a r e a e s c n o e d α» ov ρ ^ live, so that it is not necessary exclude the outside v/orta. g : » p 1 5 g o i n g out o, the the 4 c a s e s m this 3 · . o u p ° e n c „ u r a g e l i s s f ^ s n s . because o, the parents' own timidity. b i r r e d κ school £ had i.q. s below 80 i h e y ^ ^ . children .aware of hei o/m ^ who attempt to ,cover ρ ' " b l e m . a grandiose solution to the pre ^ group l ^ j j o i n i n g the * d ^ mond (diagram 19). j h e reia diamond! a n d _faeld^on the conclusion: b e c a u s e of the small n u m b e m n e c ^ h group i u r t h e r investigation may b e n r e & s a y field of projective f ^ ^ ^ i i s o n a l i t y that relationships d o e r i s t b e t w e e pe ^ patterns a n d response j ^ t t e m s fn the ball-and-hela test. the u s a s s i s t a n c e ^ o f i^^erpretaaon may be ^ ^ s k d ^ ^ h m g between f a i l u ^ u e ment. s p o n s o i : sir ernest oppenheimer to whom we express out sincere thanks αιαπιυικα --, test is quite fortuuoiu, b ^ ,, h t obliged to j o i n tne two. this is ^ reaction, b e c a u s e in all t h s y m . of the like is towards jn® ^ | ο τ π 1 3 d f the bolism m a y ' b e u n d e r s t o o d ^ ^ m * ^ family background ο t n e . e cnu r_ c a m e from home where l " e r ® n t s i i n group 18—miscellaneous could only b e classified a s un<=lci* all this group has in c o m m o n ^ m ^ patterns a r e unlike all j ™ ® r ^ a r d s s i s t i c " ^ u ' o c ' r a s it i s possible that a n d 2 a s { r o m ^ m . the exceptional pattern ι sistence on doing things in o n . s or from social non-conformity. the finest selection i of speech and music i appreciation records for children 1 are always available on all speeds at 'the connoisseurs record shop5 i t & virf ^ fe p f * © s ® & g r a m o p h o n e 1 0 3 a eloff street p h o n e : 2 2 1 6 2 5 po. box 2400 22-5445 j o h a n n e s b u r g r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abstract introduction quantifiers research design and method results discussion conclusion acknowledgements references about the author(s) joanine nel department of general linguistics, stellenbosch university, south africa frenette southwood department of general linguistics, stellenbosch university, south africa citation nel, j., & southwood f. (2016). the comprehension and production of quantifiers in isixhosa-speaking grade 1 learners. south african journal of communication disorders, 63(2), a138. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i2.138 original research the comprehension and production of quantifiers in isixhosa-speaking grade 1 learners joanine nel, frenette southwood received: 03 aug. 2015; accepted: 10 feb. 2016; published: 20 may 2016 copyright: © 2016. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract background: quantifiers form part of the discourse-internal linguistic devices that children need to access and produce narratives and other classroom discourse. little is known about the development especially the prodiction of quantifiers in child language, specifically in speakers of an african language. objectives: the study aimed to ascertain how well grade 1 isixhosa first language (l1) learners perform at the beginning and at the end of grade 1 on quantifier comprehension and production tasks. method: two low socioeconomic groups of l1 isixhosa learners with either isixhosa or english as language of learning and teaching (lolt) were tested in february and november of their grade 1 year with tasks targeting several quantifiers. results: the isixhosa lolt group comprehended no/none, any and all fully either in february or then in november of grade 1, and they produced all assessed quantifiers in february of grade 1. for the english lolt group, neither the comprehension nor the production of quantifiers was mastered by the end of grade 1, although there was a significant increase in both their comprehension and production scores. conclusion: the english lolt group made significant progress in comprehension and production of quantifiers, but still performed worse than peers who had their l1 as lolt. generally, children with no or very little prior knowledge of the lolt need either, (1) more deliberate exposure to quantifier-rich language or, (2) longer exposure to general classroom language before quantifiers can be expected to be mastered sufficiently to allow access to quantifier-related curriculum content. introduction although language acquisition is fast and efficient until the age of approximately five years, becoming a proficient speaker of one’s mother tongue is an extended process which continues up to approximately the age of nine years (berman, 2004). later-developing language skills include the ability to use low-frequency syntactic structures such as passive constructions, subordinate clauses, and low-frequency adverbial conjunctions; past perfect marking and modal auxiliaries (nippold, 2004); and noun phrase elaboration (khorounjaia & tolchinsky, 2004). quantifiers, a further later-developing syntactic category, form part of the discourse-internal linguistic devices children need to (1) contrast and differentiate characters and objects within narratives and other spoken and written texts and (2) describe quantities in mathematical literacy. little is known about the development of quantifiers in child language, especially the production thereof, and particularly in children who speak an african language. this study investigates the comprehension and production of quantifiers by young school-going speakers with isixhosa as mother tongue. young children in south africa generally have low literacy levels (department of basic education, 2014; olivier, 2009), which are linked to poor language skills (klop & tuomi, 2007), specifically to an inability to comprehend and produce some of the above mentioned later-developing constructions used by the children’s teachers in the classroom. upon entering school, children are exposed to classroom discourse, which (1) comprises more formal spoken discourse and writing, (2) is often decontextualised in nature (see naremore, densmore & harman, 1995), and (3) contains complex syntax (see dunn davison et al. 2012). children acquire greater syntactic proficiency if they are exposed to linguistic input frequently containing multiclausal utterances rather than to simplified speech (huttenlocher, vasilyeva, cymerman & levine, 2002). there is thus an ‘on-going, cyclical relationship between literacy and later language development …, a process that is heavily supported by … [language input]’ (nippold, 2004, p. 6). apart from linguistic input, language and literacy skills are also affected by socioeconomic status (ses). children growing up in low ses environments may be poverty-situated in terms of not only their physical conditions but also their development of language and literacy skills (aram & biron, 2004; farran, 1982; klop & tuomi, 2007). tough (1982) states that ‘children from disadvantaged sections of the community [do not] generally lack language but their expectations about using language do not support learning’ (p. 13). the language and socialisation style to which children are exposed, (1) influence the type of language that the child will finally master, and (2) propagate certain information-processing strategies that affect later learning (farran, 1982). children from low ses backgrounds who have their first language (l1) as their language of learning and teaching (lolt) may thus be disadvantaged when compared to their middle class peers in terms of literacy attainment because of the type of language to which the former group receives exposure. however, children from low ses backgrounds who have their second or third language as lolt may be at an even greater disadvantage. the reason for this is that poverty-situated children with a non-l1 as lolt receive language input of inferior quality (lacking certain complex language structures) but also of inferior quantity (huttenlocher, vasilyeva, waterfall, vevea & hedges, 2007). research question we report here on an investigation of the comprehension and production of quantifiers by isixhosa-speaking grade 1 learners in schools situated in low ses areas who have either isixhosa or english as lolt. the following research questions were posed: what progress do these learners make in quantifier comprehension and production during their grade 1 year? how, if at all, does the answer to research question 1 differ for the english lolt and the isixhosa lolt groups? study rationale nippold (2004) concludes that the more that is learned about the nature of later language development (such as investigated in the current study), the relation it has to literacy, and the factors which underlie its growth, the more insight the researcher will gain into the difficulties that children encounter with language in the school context. such difficulties may manifest not only in children with specific or other language impairment, but also in children who develop typically according to the norms of their community but find themselves in contexts in which the quality and quantity of their linguistic input are not ideal for optimal language and literacy development. quantifiers quantifiers defined a quantifier is a word or short phrase which indicates the amount or quantity of an object that is referred to by the noun phrase which the quantifier modifies (southwood & van dulm, 2012a). the format of this quantification must allow a distinction between properties, on the one hand, and individuals possessing these properties, on the other (braine & o’brien, 1998; brooks & sekerina, 2005/2006; o’brien et al. 2003). quantifiers are functional categories (radford, 2004) lacking specific descriptive content. as such, they can modify any semantic noun class where grammatical restrictions do not prohibit such modification. because quantifiers modify noun phrases and determine the quantificational properties of noun expressions, quantifiers generally act as a type of determiner (radford, 2001). beghelli and stowell (1997) state that quantifiers can be categorised in several manners. radford (2004), for instance, categorises quantifiers as universal, existential, or partitive. universal quantifiers are defined as ‘free-choice’ quantifiers such as all/both (radford, 2009). in contrast, the meaning of existential quantifiers relates to the existence of some entity. for instance, some in there is some coffee in the pot refers to coffee that actually exists (unlike any in is there any coffee left?, which questions the existence of coffee) (radford, 2009). partitive quantifiers quantify part of the members of a given set, as some in some children like broccoli or any in do any children like broccoli? (radford, 2009). roeper (2007) distinguishes between collective and distributive quantifiers, where all is collective and every distributive. quantifiers can also be either prenominal (occurring before the noun, as in do you have any books?) or pronominal (standing on their own, as in do you have any?). both prenominal and pronominal quantifiers can occur in either the subject or the object position of a sentence. beghelli and stowell (1997) base their categorisation of quantifiers on the syntax of quantifier scope. scope pertains to the referential dependencies between the quantifier phrase and the clause in which it occurs (beghelli, 1993). for instance, the meaning of every is ‘all possible’, but every (1) has scope over the subject of the sentence in every boy sees the dog, (2) has scope over the object in the boy sees every dog, and (3) assumes scopal ambiguity in every boy sees a dog, where either every boy sees the same dog or every boy sees a different dog. the development of quantifiers in child language quantifier development has been studied intensively, and the relevant theoretical developments and empirical studies are rich and varied. in table 1, we provide a summary of the available findings on the age of acquisition of specific quantifiers. note that there is information available on quantifier acquisition in one of the languages relevant to the current study (english), but not on the other (isixhosa). afrikaans data are provided alongside english data in table 1 to show that there are differences in age of acquisition between these two typologically similar languages. it could thus be assumed that there will also be differences between english and isixhosa, which are typologically dissimilar. below the table, we provide a brief exposition of how the quantifiers in the table are formed in english and in isixhosa. table 1: age of acquisition of quantifiers per meaning and scope. no literature on the production of these quantifiers could be traced for afrikaans, apart from southwood and van dulm (2012a), which states that production of quantifiers by afrikaans-speaking children is mastered only after the age of 9 years. there is also a dearth of literature on child l2 acquisition of quantifiers. available studies (e.g. lakshmanan, 1995) focus on theoretical and psycholinguistic phenomena and how these would affect children’s developing l2 grammars, but there are little empirical data on child l2 acquisition of quantifiers. how quantifiers present themselves in english and isixhosa quantifiers in english english quantifiers include but are not limited to every, all, any, many/more/most, no/none, and some and are invariant forms which do not undergo any inflection. they can be prenominal as in all students are welcome or pronominal as in all are welcome (radford, 2001), but not all quantifiers can be used both prenominally and pronominally in english; for example, pronominal every is grammatical (as in every student wants to graduate) but pronominal every not (as in *every wants to graduate) (radford, 2001). some english prenominal quantifiers can occur as one-word units (when occurring with nouns, as in many people are relieved) or as part of constructions (when occurring with nouns or pronouns, as in many of the people/them are relieved). quantifiers can have generic reference, and then the zero article is used (as in all men are handsome) or specific reference, and then the definite article with or without of is used (as in all (of) the men are handsome). quantifiers in isixhosa the isixhosa quantifiers -nke ‘every/all’, -nye ‘some’, -phi ‘any’, -ninzi ‘many’ and -ngaphezulu ‘more’ have variant forms where different inflectional processes derive the quantifier, in contrast to inkoliso ‘most’ and a‘no/none’ which are invariant and where only a single morpheme acts as the quantifier. the quantifiers -nke ‘every’ and a‘none’ illustrate this difference between variance and invariance: the quantifier stem -nke combines with the quantifier root -oand the relevant subject agreement (according to the noun class of the noun that the quantifier modifies), as in (1). however, the isixhosa equivalent of every can also be expressed by the quantifier form elowo or by a combination of the adjective stem ngaand -nye (in the case of a distributive reading), as in (2): 1. wonke umntwana uyadlala wo-nke um-ntwana u-ya-dlal-a subject morpheme. second person. quantifier root-every noun class 1-child subject morpheme 1-aspect-play-present. final vowel ‘every child is playing’ (= all children – collective) 2. umntwana ngomnye uyadlala um-ntwana nga-um-nye u-ya-dlal-a noun class 1-child preposition-noun class 1-one (every) subject morpheme 1-aspect-play-present. final vowel ‘every child is playing’ (= every single one – distributive) whereas negation in english can occur by using a quantificational modifier with the noun (as in no child), such negation in isixhosa occurs on the verb. the isixhosa equivalent of the english quantifiers no and none is expressed by adding the negative prefix ato the verb, as in (3): 3. andizifuna iiapile a-ndi-zi-fun-a ii-aplile negative-subject morpheme.first person-object morpheme 10-want-present. final vowel noun class 10-apples‘ i want no apples’/‘i do not want apples’ research design and method design the current study forms part of a larger project on later-developing language skills in young school-going afrikaans-, english-, and isixhosa-speaking children (see nel, 2014). in this study, the comprehension and production of quantifiers at the beginning and end of the grade 1 year were assessed amongst isixhosa-speaking children from two schools, one with english as lolt and the other with isixhosa as lolt. data were collected in the lolt of the learners. the study was empirical and had a longitudinal and cross-sectional design: the nature of research question 1 (which asks what development in the comprehension and production of quantifiers takes place between the start and the end of grade 1) lends itself to a longitudinal design, by examining changes that occur over the course of a school year. in the cross-sectional part of this study, participants were grouped according to lolt to see if and how the level of mastery of quantifiers differs between these two groups (to answer research question 2). data were collected with an action research approach. the latter involves a cyclic process in which researchers follow a series of steps that include planning, observing, and evaluating the effects of a specific action which is to be researched (gray, 2004). participating schools and participants the study had to be conducted in two similar schools attended by l1 isixhosa-speaking learners, one with english as lolt and the other with isixhosa. the selection criteria for the schools were as follows: situated in communities with low ses a national quintile of 3 or lower (i.e. non-fee-paying schools) the same geographical classification (either both rural or both urban). we approached several schools about participating in the study. the only two that consented to participate also met the selection criteria. the english lolt school is parallel medium, with an afrikaans and an english stream. it is situated in a rural area 10 km from the nearest town centre. its 923 learners live on the surrounding farms and in various nearby informal settlements. the school has one educator per 34 learners and a national quintile of 1. the participants from this school (14 male; 16 female) had a mean age of 6.6 years (range 6.0 years – 7.6 years) at the first point of data collection. of the 30 english lolt participants, 21 were exposed to only isixhosa in their homes, one to isixhosa and isizulu, seven to isixhosa and english, and one to isixhosa, english, and afrikaans. thirteen of the english lolt participants were born in stellenbosch and never moved away. the region in which they had received input and in which language acquisition had taken place has thus been stable and homogenous in comparison to nine other participants. (no data were available on the remaining eight participants.) of those nine participants, four were born in cape town and their parents later moved to stellenbosch, whereas three were born in johannesburg and another two in the eastern cape. the ninth participant grew up in the western cape, but it is not specified where. the place of birth plays an important role, because it means that the regional languages and language varieties to which the participants may have been exposed are heterogeneous, and this might affect the characteristics of their language. unfortunately, there was no indication of the age at which the children’s families moved to stellenbosch. in terms of the exposure to other languages outside of the home, the english lolt participants formed a heterogeneous group: 18 attended a playschool or pre-grade 1 educational facility (21 facilities in total) during their early childhood years; no data on this were available for the remaining 12 participants. for seven participants, isixhosa was the language of instruction during their preschool years, for two english, for one afrikaans, for another one a combination of english and isixhosa, for three a combination of afrikaans, english, and isixhosa and for one a combination of afrikaans and english. no data on language of instruction was available for the remaining three of those 18 participants who attended a preschool facility. the isixhosa lolt school is in a township adjacent to the industrial area of the same town, 3.4 km from the town centre. it is one of two primary schools in the township. it has 1494 learners and one educator per 40 learners. its national quintile ranking is 1. at the first point of data collection, the mean age of the participants from this school (15 male; 16 female) was 6.8 years (range 6.0 – 8.11 years). of the 31 isixhosa lolt participants, only one was exposed to another language in addition to isixhosa at home, namely to afrikaans. sixteen of the isixhosa lolt participants were born in stellenbosch and never moved away. four of the other 15 were born in cape town and their parents later moved to stellenbosch whereas one was born in tygerberg hospital (which means that the parents lived somewhere in the western cape province at that time) and one in the eastern cape. another participant was born in the western cape but the specific place was not indicated. the children who were not born in stellenbosch will have language profiles that look dissimilar to those children who have stayed in the stellenbosch area their whole lives. it was however not indicated at which time in the children’s lives their families moved to stellenbosch. for 8 of the participants, it was not indicated where they were born. approximately a quarter of the children in the isixhosa lolt group had not attended a playschool during early childhood. twenty-three of the 31 participants attended a total of 17 different institutions, with some overlap between those institutions attended by the english and isixhosa lolt groups. the range of language input and the exposure to languages in the isixhosa group in these pre-gr 1 educational settings was wide: nine participants received input in isixhosa only, another nine in english and isixhosa, and one in english only. data collection material the quantifiers booklet of the receptive and expressive activities for language therapy (realt; southwood & van dulm, 2012b) served as data collection instrument. the realt material was designed for use as language intervention material with children from four to nine years who exhibit a language delay or have a language disorder, but can be used as an informal language assessment instrument (southwood & van dulm, 2012a). its authors also state that it is suitable for l2 speakers of english as well as for children from poverty-situated communities which can profit from directed language stimulation to aid with language development (southwood & van dulm, 2012a). since its publication, the english version was translated to isixhosa by the realt authors and these two versions were then used for data collection in this study. the quantifier section of the realt includes six different quantifiers: all (isixhosa: -nke), any (-phi), every (-nke/nga-+-nye), many/more/most (-ninzi/-ngaphezulu/inkoliso), no/none (a-) and some (-nye). four quantifiers (all/-nke, every/-nke/-nga-+-nye, no/none/a-, and some/-nye) have one set of a comprehension items and another set of production items, whereas any/-phi and many/more/most/-ninzi/-ngaphezulu/inkoliso have comprehension items only. each of these sets, apart from those of no/none/a-, has two subsets, one targeting quantifier meaning (five items each for comprehension and three for production) and another targeting quantifier scope (again five and three items for comprehension and production, respectively). the meaning items comprise stimulus questions based on a picture, requiring either yes/no or other one-word responses. the scope items comprise a picture selection task in which learners are presented with a verbal stimulus to which they respond by choosing that one out of three pictures that matches the stimulus. the realt assesses production of only every/-nke/nga-+-nye, all/-nke, some/-nye, and no/none/aas appropriate picture material could not be generated for any/ -phi and many/more/most/-ninzi/-ngaphezulu/inkoliso (southwood & van dulm, 2012a). every/-nke/nga-+-nye, all/-nke, and some/-nye each has six production items, of which the first three assess the meaning and the last three the scope of these quantifiers. the scope items differ in format from the meaning items, in that there are two parts to each scope item, namely an (a)-part which aims to elicit a response containing a quantifier with a specific scope, and a (b)-part which aims to elicit the same quantifier but with contrasting scope. the quantifier no/none/ahas only five production items which appear in a question answering task. data collection and analysis data were collected from each learner individually in his or her lolt by four fieldworkers. the quantifiers booklet of the realt was administered to 61 isixhosa-speaking grade 1 learners (30 with english and 31 with isixhosa as lolt) at the beginning of the first term of 2013 (henceforth also ‘february’). these learners were reassessed in the same manner in the fourth term of 2013 (henceforth also ‘november’). responses were documented on paper scoresheets. where spontaneous revisions occurred, the participant’s last response was recorded. if a targeted response was given, the fieldworker made encouraging remarks such as ‘good job!’ or ‘well done’. in the case of an incorrect or non-target response, the fieldworker followed up with a somewhat more elaborate version of the initial stimulus. correct responses to follow-up were scored as correct. no further opportunities for correct response were provided after the one follow-up. responses were then transferred from the scoresheets to custom designed excel sheets. the following were calculated separately for comprehension and production of each quantifier, with a score of 90% or more taken as an indication of mastery: the percentage of responses correct for each subtype per learner the average of these percentages for each subtype per lolt group the average percentage of all comprehension subtypes and of all production subtypes collectively per lolt group. the wilcoxon matched pairs test was used to compare the data collected at the beginning and at the end of the year for each lolt group separately, with p < 0.05 as significance level. the direction of significance (whether scores were better in february or in november) was determined by means of inspecting box and whiskers plots. the data for the english lolt group and the isixhosa lolt group were also compared to each other, in order to answer research question 2. this was done by means of the mann–whitney u test (with continuity correction), where english data collected in february were compared to isixhosa data collected then, and english data collected in november to isixhosa data collected then. the direction of significance was again derived from inspection of box and whisker plots. the programme used for statistical analyses was statistica 12. ethical considerations ethical clearance for the study was granted by the research ethics committee (humanities) of stellenbosch university (protocol number gl010812). permission to conduct the study was obtained from the principals and relevant teachers at the two study schools. informed consent was obtained from participants’ parents or legal guardians by distributing information letters and consent forms to them via the school. the letters explained in plain language and in the lolt of the particular school the general purpose of the study, the procedures to be followed during data collection and reporting of the findings, and the voluntary nature of participation. participants gave written assent after the fieldworkers had explained to them in isixhosa what research is and what tasks they would be expected to perform. participants and their parents or guardians were also informed that participation could be terminated at any point without them having to provide a reason for doing so or being penalised in any manner. confidentiality and anonymity were assured during all stages of the research process, amongst others by assigning participant codes instead of using participant names. reliability and validity the following measures were taken in order to increase the reliability and validity of the obtained data: the realt was administered exactly as stipulated in the manual (southwood & van dulm, 2012a). data were collected by a small number of fieldworkers (two per school), with no changes in school assignment between the beginning and the end of the year. all fieldworkers received the same intensive training on, amongst others, the research protocol and task administration and scoring. during data collection, responses were recorded and scored on the scoresheets in real time (directly after being given). each non-target response was written down verbatim. at the end of each data collection day, the first author verified the accuracy of the scoring. any ambiguity or inconsistency was cleared up with the fieldworker before the next data collection day. accuracy of data transfer to the excel sheet was checked. results quantifier comprehension english lolt group participants who had english as lolt did not obtain a 90% score on the comprehension tasks (meaning and scope) for any of the six quantifiers tested, neither in february nor in november, apart from on the comprehension task for meaning of no/none. in february, the average comprehension score for meaning of no/none was 76%, but in november it was 97% (table 2). despite the general lack of mastery, scores for comprehension of meaning increased statistically significantly from february to november, with the exception of some which remained the same (table 2). regarding scores for comprehension of scope, only every and many/more/most showed a statistically significant increase (table 2). table 2: english lolt and isixhosa lolt quantifier comprehension scores: descriptive statistics. isixhosa lolt group in the isixhosa lolt group, comprehension scores for the meaning subsets of a‘no/none’ and -phi ‘any’ indicated mastery in february, with a further increase in november. the meaning subset of -nke ‘all’ was fully acquired at the end of grade 1: the average percentage increased from 89% in february to 95% in november. the score for the meaning subset of -nke/nga-+-nye ‘every’ was 90% in february but decreased slightly to 88% in november. this quantifier was thus almost fully acquired during grade 1. the remaining quantifiers in the meaning subset had high average percentages and were almost fully acquired in november (table 2). the scores of the scope subset decreased slightly from the beginning to the end of the year, but this decrease was not statistically significant. the scores for the scope subset are not as high as those for the meaning subset, and comprehension of the scope of the assessed quantifiers were not mastered by the end of g1. quantifier production english lolt group in the english lolt group, quantifier production errors mainly included quantifier omission (such as the boys are kicking the balls instead of all the boys are kicking the balls), the use of definite articles instead of a quantifier (such as they picked the flowers instead of they picked some flowers), circumlocutions with negation (such as i don’t see red instead of there are no red balloons) and irrelevant picture description. no quantifiers were fully acquired by november as none of the average percentages were above 90, but scores for both the meaning and the scope subsets of all the quantifiers showed an increase from february to november. not all increases were statistically significant though (table 3). for the meaning subset of every, all and some, there was a statistically significant increase but not for no/none. for the scope subset, only the quantifier all showed a significant increase. both in february and november, meaning scores were higher than scope scores (table 3). table 3: english lolt and isixhosa lolt quantifier production scores: descriptive statistics. isixhosa lolt group quantifier production errors made by the isixhosa lolt participants mainly comprised quantifier omission, circumlocutions with negation (umtwana omnye akanaye unonkala ‘the other child doesn’t have a crab’ instead of oononkala bahamba ezinyaweni zabanye babantwana ‘the crabs are running over some of the children’s feet’), and irrelevant picture description or other irrelevant responses (such as bahamba ngenyawo ‘walking barefoot’ instead of abanye oononkala baqabela ngaphaya kweenyawo zabantwana ‘some crabs are running over the children’s feet’). the isixhosa lolt participants had production scores for meaning and for scope of 90% or higher in february and in november for all quantifiers. meaning and scope scores were almost similar in february and again in november. the increases or decreases from february to november show no specific pattern, and none of the differences between february and november were significant apart from the score for the scope subset of -nye ‘some’ which showed a significant decrease. despite this decrease, the november score was still above 90% (table 3). comparison of english lolt and isixhosa lolt scores a comparison between the english and the isixhosa lolt learners shows a clear distinction between these two groups in terms of both their comprehension and production skills, with the learners who have their mother tongue as lolt consistently obtaining higher scores (80% – 95%) than those who have their l2 as lolt (50% – 60%). for each quantifier, subset, and set, there was a statistically significant difference between the two lolt groups, apart from comprehension of the meaning of no/none/a(p = 0.507) and production of the meaning of all/-nke (p = 0.075). in these latter two cases, the scores of the isixhosa lolt group were however still higher than those of the english lolt group; see figure 1 which is a box and whiskers plot of the total scores for comprehension and production for the two lolt groups. table 4 is a summary of the ages of mastery of comprehension and production of the quantifiers assessed in the current study. figure 1: the comprehension and production total scores of all quantifier types (%) per language of learning and teaching. table 4: age of acquisition of comprehension and production of quantifiers based on data from the current study discussion based on the available literature on the age of acquisition of quantifiers (table 1), quantifiers seem to be early developing rather than later-developing, with the exception of the quantifier every. however, a distinction between meaning and scope is not always made in the relevant literature. from the data obtained in the current study, it is not conclusive whether quantifiers are earlieror later-developing in isixhosa l1 speakers: comprehension of a‘no/none’ and -phi ‘any’ has been mastered by the beginning of grade 1 (age 6.1 – 8.11) by isixhosa-speaking children, which could indicate early development. however, comprehension of the remaining quantifiers is only mastered later, pointing to later development. different quantifiers are thus mastered at different ages. this could be because of differences in the inflectional processes involved in the various isixhosa quantifiers. also, the complex processes which a child has to apply in order to interpret a quantified phrase correctly might play a role in why some quantifiers prove to be more difficult for learners than others quantifiers. in this regard, brooks and sekerina (2005/2006) state that: although quantifiers play a very important role in logical reasoning …, their acquisition may be delayed relative to other sorts of lexical items (e.g. nouns and verbs) because their complex patterns of usage often result in interpretive ambiguities. (p. 177) despite the fact that the isixhosa lolt group had not yet acquired the comprehension of -nke/nga-+-nye ‘every’, -nke ‘all’, -nye ‘some’, and -ninzi/-ngaphezulu/inkoliso ‘many/more/most’ by the end of grade 1, quantifier production is already fully acquired at beginning of grade 1. a possible reason why the production scores are higher than the comprehension scores could be that the production items, unlike the comprehension items, do not provide opposer and distracter pictures which could confuse the learners. as stated above, errors in the production data comprised the omission of a quantifier, circumlocutions, irrelevant picture description, and responses completely unrelated to the stimulus, but not substitution of one quantifier with another. the learners thus did not produce untargeted quantifiers as there was no stimulus priming other quantifiers. based on the results of this study, one can conclude that isixhosa grade 1 learners who have isixhosa as lolt have sufficient command of quantifiers when they enter grade 1 to allow them to understand classroom discourse containing this syntactic category. turning to the isixhosa-speaking learners with their l2 as lolt: none of these learners achieved a percentage correct score of 90% or more for quantifier comprehension (not even at the end of their grade 1 year), and their scores were significantly lower than those of their peers with isixhosa as lolt. despite the fact that the english lolt learners had various ages of first exposure to english, ranging from no exposure prior to entering grade 1 to exposure from birth or preschool entrance, none of them had mastered quantifier comprehension at school entry, and all required more than one year’s exposure to english classroom discourse to master quantifier comprehension. note however that there was a significant increase in the comprehension and production scores of english quantifiers from the beginning to the end of grade 1. the increase in the english lolt group’s scores was statistically significant. thus, despite the fact that these quantifiers are not yet fully acquired by the end of grade 1, significant development takes place between the beginning and the end of grade 1. in this regard, jordaan (2011) found that l2 learners who are integrated with l1 language learners catch up with these l1 language learners by grade 3. jordaan (2011) also states that although those l2 learners who are not integrated with l1 peers do make significant progress by the end of grade 3 on most of the language skills that she assessed, they do not reach the same level as those integrated into a l1 learning context. the learners in the english lolt group of the current study find themselves in the latter situation: they are l2 learners of english in a class consisting of mainly l2 learners of english. they have received and still receive limited english input, because their main (and, in many cases, only) source of english input is their teachers who are not english l1 speakers. although these learners are supposed to be taught through medium of english only (as per the language policy of the school), their teachers are ‘forced’ to code switch in class (between english and isixhosa) because of the learners’ low level of english comprehension and production at the beginning and, to a large extent, still at the end of grade 1 (personal communication with teachers). the school’s language policy is thus not always followed in practice, as it is not practical (or even possible) in all teaching contexts. this group of isixhosa-speaking learners is at a disadvantage compared to those in a classroom with isixhosa as lolt, because in the absence of code switched classroom discourse, they have limited ability to access complex english sentences such as those containing quantifiers. by contrast, isixhosa-speaking learners receiving their tuition in their l1 will be adequately prepared to understand quantifiers in the narratives and numerical literacy tasks they encounter in the grade 1 classroom. conclusion the study investigated the quantifier comprehension and production of children from low ses backgrounds who consequently might not receive adequate linguistic input at home for the acquisition of complex language constructions. when comparing the scores of these low ses learners with those obtained by the higher ses learners assessed by southwood and van dulm (2012a), it is clear that the age of acquisition is later for the low ses isixhosa l1 speakers who receive their schooling in their l1 and even more so for those who receive their schooling in english. children who are situated in low ses circumstances are generally impoverished in terms of the language input they receive, and the language development norms applicable to middle or high ses communities cannot necessarily be applied to low ses communities. this study rendered limited normative data on the acquisition of quantifier constructions by child speakers of isixhosa and contributed to the still small pool of normative data on the language acquisition of older children. in summary in this regard, there seems to be a general acquisition order for the meaning subset: -phi ‘any’ and a‘no/none’ are acquired earlier than -nke ‘all’, and -ninzi/-ngaphezulu/inkoliso ‘many/more/most’, -nye ‘some’, and nke/nga-+-nye ‘every’ are acquired thereafter. the findings of this study support the notion that child language acquisition has not been completed by the age of five years, thus challenging certain assumptions about language acquisition and developmental norms. despite the fact that children can generally construct most sentence types and decode complex semantic relationships in their l1 upon school entry, their language continues to develop in significant manners during at least their first years of school. the study had several limitations. these included a limited number study schools in only one geographical location; group generalisations instead of careful consideration of possible individual variation; combining the results of the two genders instead of searching for possible gender-related differences; considering grade instead of chronological age; and focusing on learner responses only instead of imbedding the study in classroom observations so as to ascertain the potential influence of teacher talk and pedagogical styles on learners’ linguistic knowledge. despite these limitations of this study, the findings have a practical implication: children with no or very little prior knowledge of their lolt will generally need either, (1) more deliberate exposure to quantifier rich language in their lolt, or (2) longer exposure to general classroom discourse in their lolt before quantifiers can be expected to be mastered sufficiently to allow access to quantifier-related curriculum content. without such exposure, children from low ses backgrounds for whom there is a mismatch between their l1 and lolt are unlikely to function optimally in the grade 1 classroom. acknowledgements the authors thank the staff at the department of african languages of stellenbosch university and dr mantoa smouse for assistance with the description of quantifiers in isixhosa and with the analysis of the isixhosa data. the authors also thank the fieldworkers who assisted with data collection. this material is based on work financially supported by the national research foundation. any opinions, findings, conclusions, or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the authors and therefore the nrf does not accept any liability in regard thereto. competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships which may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions the study on which the article is based formed part of j.n.’s (stellenbosch university) phd project (of which f.s. [stellenbosch university] was the main supervisor), which in turn formed part of a larger project on the language acquisition of afrikaans-, englishand/or isixhosa-speaking children, with f.s. as principal investigator. the study was conceptualised by f.s., but j.n. conducted the study (obtained ethical clearance, permissions, consent and assent; trained field workers; and collected, analysed, and interpreted the data). the first version of this article was drafted by j.n. and subsequent versions were a collaborative effort between the two authors. references aram, d., & biron, s. (2004). joint storybook reading and joint writing interventions among low ses preschoolers: differential contributions to early literacy. early childhood research quarterly, 19(4), 588–610. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ecresq.2004.10.003 beghelli, f. (1993). a minimalist approach to quantifier scope. proceedings of the north east linguistics society, 23(1), 65–80. beghelli, f., & stowell, t. (1997). distributivity and negation: the syntax of each and every. in a. szabolcsi (ed.), ways of scope taking, (pp. 71–107). dordrecht: kluwer. berman, r.a. (2004). between emergence and mastery: the long developmental route of language acquisition. in r.a. berman (ed.), language development across childhood and adolescence, (pp. 9–34). amsterdam: john benjamins publishing company. braine, m.d.s., & o’brien, d.p. (1998). mental logic. mahwah, nj: lawrence erlbaum. brooks, p., & braine, m.d.s. (1996). what do children know about the universal quantifiers all and each? cognition, 60, 235–268. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0010-0277(96)00712-3 brooks, p.j., & sekerina, i. (2005/2006). shortcuts to quantifier interpretation in children and adults. language acquisition, 13(3), 177–206. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la1303_2 department of basic education. (2014). report on the annual national assessments of 2014. retrieved july 31, 2015, from http://www.saqa.org.za/docs/rep_annual/2014/report%20on%20the%20ana%20of%202014.pdf dunn davison, m., schuele, m.m., fisher, j., combs, s., krimm, h., & dickinson, d. (2012). preschool teacher talk: what relates to more complex syntax. poster presented at the asha convention, atlanta, ga. retrieved april 2, 2015, from http://www.mc.vanderbilt.edu/documents/cll/files/dunn%20davidson%20et%20al.pdf farran, d.c. (1982). mother-child interaction, language development, and the school performance of poverty children. in l. feagans, & d.c. farran (eds.), the language of children reared in poverty. implications for evaluation and intervention (pp. 19–52). educational psychology series. new york: academic press inc. foppolo, f., guasti, m.t., & chierchia, g. (2012). scalar implicatures in child language: give children a chance. language learning and development, 8(4), 365–394. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15475441.2011.626386 gathercole, v. (1985). he has too much hard questions: the acquisition of the linguistic mass-count distinction in much and many. journal of child language, 12, 395–415. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000900006504 gray, d.e. (2004). doing research in the real world. london: sage publications ltd. halberda, j., taing, l., & lidz, j. (2008). the development of ‘most’ comprehension and its potential dependence on counting ability in preschoolers. language learning and development, 4(2), 99–121. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/15475440801922099. hanlon, c.c. (1987). acquisition of set-relational quantifiers in early childhood. genetic, social and general psychology monographs, 113(2), 215–264. huang, a., & crain, s. (2014). acquisition of the polarity sensitive item renhe ‘any’ in mandarin chinese. journal of child language, 41, 861–889. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000913000275 huttenlocher, j., vasilyeva, m., cymerman, e., & levine, s.c. (2002). language input at home and at school: relation to syntax. cognitive psychology, 45(3), 337–374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0285(02)00500-5 huttenlocher, j., vasilyeva, m., waterfall, h.r., vevea, j.l., & hedges, l.v. (2007). the varieties of speech to children. developmental psychology, 43, 1062–1083. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0012-1649.43.5.1062 jordaan, h. (2011). delving into the development of academic language by foundation phase english language learners. doctoral dissertation. university of the witwatersrand. katsos, n. (2009). the acquisition of quantification: insights from sli and cross-linguistic research. paper presented at the third meeting of cost action iso 804, newcastle. khorounjaia, e., & tolchinsky, l. (2004). discursive constraints on the lexical realization of arguments in spanish. in r.a. berman (ed.), language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 83–109). amsterdam: john benjamins publishing company. klop, d., & tuomi, s.k. (2007). the persistence of language disorders in a group of disadvantaged grade 3 learners. the south african journal of communication disorders, 54, 59–66. lakshmanan, u. (1995). child second language acquisition of syntax. studies in second language acquisition, 17, 301–329. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0272263100014224 naremore, r.c., densmore, a.e., & harman, d.r. (1995). language intervention with school-aged children. conversation, narrative, and text. san diego, ca: singular publishing group. nel, j.h. (2014). the comprehension and production of later developing language constructions by afrikaans-, english and isixhosa-speaking grade 1 learners. doctoral dissertation. stellenbosch: stellenbosch university. retrieved may 27, 2015, from http://scholar.sun.ac.za/handle/10019.1/97144 nippold, m.a. (2004). research on later language development: international perspectives. in r. a. berman (ed.), language development across childhood and adolescence (pp. 1–8). amsterdam: john benjamins publishing company. o’brien, d.p., roazzi, a., athias, r.a., dias, m.g., brandão, m.c., & brooks, p.j. (2003). the language of thought and the existence of a mental logic: experimental investigations in the laboratory and in the field. psychologica, 32, 263–284. odic, d., pietroski, p., hunter, t., lidz, j., & halberda, j. (2012). young children’s understanding of ‘more’ and discrimination of number and surface area. journal of experimental psychology, 39(2), 451–461. o’leary, c. (1994). children’s awareness of polarity sensitivity. unpublished ma thesis. university of connecticut. olivier, j.m. (2009). investigating literacy development among learners with a second language as medium of education. the effects of an emergent literacy stimulation program in grade r. doctoral dissertation. stellenbosch: stellenbosch university. retrieved july 31, 2015, from http://scholar.sun.ac.za/handle/10019.1/1238 papafragou, a., & musolino, j. (2003). scalar implicatures: experiments at the semantics-pragmatics interface. cognition, 86, 253–282. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0010-0277(02)00179-8 papafragou, a., & schwarz, n. (2006). most wanted. language acquisition, 13(3), 207–251. http://dx.doi.org/10.1207/s15327817la1303_3 philip, w. (1995). event quantification in the acquisition of universal quantification. doctoral dissertation. amherst, ma, university of massachusetts. radford, a. (2001). syntax: a minimalist introduction. cambridge, uk: cambridge university press. radford, a. (2004). english syntax. an introduction. cambridge, uk: cambridge university press. radford, a. (2009). analysing english sentences: a minimalist approach. cambridge: cambridge university press. roeper, t. (2007). the prism of grammar. how child language illustrates humanism. cambridge, ma: the mit press. roeper, t., & matthei, e. (1974). on the acquisition of “all” and “some”. papers and reports on child language development, 9, 63–74. roeper, t., strauss, u., & pearson, b.z. (2006). the acquisition path of the determiner quantifier every: two kinds of spreading. in t. heizmann (ed.), papers in language acquisition (pp. 97–128), university of massachusetts occasional papers 34. amherst, ma: glsa. retrieved july 31, 2015, from http://www.umass.edu/aae/pathacq_roeperstrausspearson1115-2.pdf song, m.s. (2003). the first and second language acquisition of negative polarity items in english and korean. unpublished doctoral dissertation. university of hawai’i. southwood, f., & van dulm, o. (2012a). receptive and expressive activities for language therapy: user manual. johannesburg: jvr psychometrics. southwood, f., & van dulm, o. (2012b). receptive and expressive activities for language therapy: quantifiers. johannesburg: jvr psychometrics. stickney, h. (2003). investigations into children’s acquisition of ‘most’. unpublished manuscript. amherst, ma: university of massachusetts. thornton, r. (1995). children’s negative questions: a production/comprehension asymmetry. in j. fuller, h. han, & d. parkinson (eds.), proceedings of escol (pp. 306–317). ithaca, ny: cornell university press. tough, j. (1982). language, poverty, and disadvantage in school. in l. feagans & d.c. farran (eds.), the language of children reared in poverty. implications for evaluation and intervention (pp. 3–18). educational psychology series. new york: academic press inc. today and tomorrow in the education of the deaf* s. r i c h a r d s i l v e r m a n , ph.d. director, central institute for the deaf, st. louis, missouri, u.s.a. summary the n e e d t o base programmes for y o u n g deaf children o n an understanding of the c o m p o s i t e of their capacities is stressed. a number of these capacities, a m o n g m a n y , are cited in regard t o sensory input and language acquisition and pertinent q u e s t i o n s associated w i t h t h e m are raised. opsomming die b e h o e f t e o m programme vir jong d o w e kinders te grond o p begrip van samestelling van hul vermoeens w o r d b e k l e m t o o n . 'n aantal van hierdie verm o e e n s , uit vele gekies, w o r d aangepas m e t betrekking tot sensoriese ontvangs en taalverwerwing en pertinente vrae wat daarmee in verband staan, w o r d geopper. author's note: it is a privilege for me to be invited to contribute to the journal of the south african speech and hearing association honoring my friend and colleague prof. pierre pienaar. i look back with pleasure and appreciation to my visit, in 1967, to south africa, arranged by prof. pienaar. the intellectual stimulation, the warm hospitality, and the new experiences made the trip a memorable one for mrs. silverman and me. i was invited to contribute an article on the education of the deaf. i suggested to the editor that i submit for consideration, my keynote presentation on the subject to the international congress on education of the deaf, in august of 1970 at stockholm. the suggestion was made for two reasons. first, the talk represented my current thoughts on the subject and second it had not appeared in the periodical literature. here it is unedited from the original version. the subject assigned me by your chairman for the opening lecture of this congress is "today and tomorrow in the education of the deaf." it appears to call for an updated version of my keynote address to the 1963 international congress in washington titled "the education of deaf children past and * keynote address to the international congress on education of the deaf, stockholm, sweden, august 17, 1970. reproduced-by-lpermission of the swedish union of teachers. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 s. richard silverman prologue."18 i hope that the omission of the "past" in the present assignment is not a deliberate rejection of the recent history of our profession and the lessons it has to teach. who knows, maybe it is an unconscious bow to the credo of rebellious youth for whom the past is irrelevant. what is more likely is that the planners wisely recognize the great abundance of stimulating and provocative activity now going on in the education of deaf children and in the interest of economy of time have chosen to focus on the significance of this activity for the future. furthermore, they rightly assume that those interested enough to commit their time, energy and means to attend the congress require no tutorial in the history of our field. we recall that the 1963 congress was organized sequentially from assessment and diagnosis of children, through their educational experiences to their economic, psychological and social accommodation to the world about them. however, reflection on activities since our last meeting, especially as they are expressed in the subjects of this congress, suggests that this necessarily brief exposition concentrate on a topic that is particularly timely today with stress on the questions it raises for tomorrow. i draw liberally on the writings of those who have given serious and sustained attention to the topic. this is the increasing emphasis on very young children. after all, they are the school children of tomorrow. this is not to say that it is the only one that merits attention or that it is unrelated to such topics as curriculum and the latter day concern for the formulation of objectives against a background of an overwhelming accumulation of knowledge, media, mental health, vocational and technical training, participation by more deaf persons in educational planning, professional training, organizational and administrative arrangements, philosophic outlook and realistic aspirations for the deaf including its relation to the "sub-culture" syndrome and a host of other problems of mutual concern. it simply expresses the opinion of your keynoter operating within the time and context assigned him. i am aware too, i hasten to point out, that the education of deaf children reflects the distinctive traditions, trends and aspirations for general education of each nation represented here. i trust, nevertheless, that these differences, whatever they may be, are put aside to consider problems singularly common to all deaf children. this audience does not need to be reminded how the encouraging progress in the identification and assessment of hearing impairment in young children has underlined the necessity and, yes, the opportunity for effective preschool programs. we all realize how important for learning and over-all development of children is the period from birth to the age of five. indirect, but importantly suggestive evidence from neurophysiology points to the substantial influence on the developing nervous system of sensory experience. and we are all familiar with the emphasis placed by psycholinguists on the notion of an optimal period for acquisition of language, particularly its structural features. the implication of all of this for parents, too, properly commands our earnest attention. i it is interesting to mention here that davis underlines the need for early treatment by suggesting a hypothesis about the etiology leading to an "aphasic", "language handicapping", "difficult learning" problem in children or whatjournal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) today and tomorrow in the education of the deaf 61 ever you choose to call them. in the latest edition of hearing and deafness7 he says: "it is postulated that the auditory system requires sensory input during the early years, particularly during the second year when the normal infant begins to learn speech, in order to complete its development, and that if this normal organization does not occur, it is more difficult to bring it about later, even though sounds are then made audible by amplification. the failure to recognize the partial hearing loss leads to false expectations and inappropriate management. the child develops what may be called a "habitual disregard" for sound as a means of communication. it makes no difference whether the hearing loss is hereditary, congenital, or acquired, but it is importanuhat the loss is present during the years when children normally learn to talk." "this interpretation is still a hypothesis, important is the clinical experience that young children in whom the partial hearing loss is detected early and who are given the benefit of early and habitual exposure to loud speech, with a hearing aid as soon as they are old enough, do develop speech and effective use of their residual hearing much better than children with similar audiograms who have not had the advantage of early auditory exposure and training. the practical implication of this hypothesis is, of course, to recognize early the auditory defect and to initiate the proper management." davis also suggests a new term. "dysmathia is a more appropriate term than congenital aphasia. dysmathia is an old greek word that means exactly what we want to say, namely, difficulty or slowness in learning. if we wish to be more specific and say difficulty in learning speech and language, we can form a new word, dyslogomathia. the familiar root logos carries the connotation of both speech and language." "in summary, true congenital aphasia based on an anatomical defect or birth injury does occur, but it is rare. dyslogomathia, based on partial sensory deprivation, is a more probable explanation in a majority of the cases in which children show some reactions to fairly loud sounds but do not spontaneously develop speech, and often have great difficulty learning speech later. further study should clarify the situation and allow us to accept, reject, or modify this working hypothesis." having agreed on the general propositions stressing the need we must ask what is a sensible program. obviously any program for tomorrow will exploit our expanding knowledge of assessment of those, capacities of children that are important for their educational management. for example, in studying auditory capacity we shall supplement, wherever possible, determination of auditory thresholds with information about perception of pattern, ability to use minimal auditory cues for "total" communication, attentiveness to sound and differential sensitivity. a pertinent experiment in our laboratory by gengel,10 studying differential sensitivity for frequency and its change with training, demonstrated a reduction in the difference limen in hearing impaired children after only limited practice. he found that a number of hearing impaired children discriminated differences of 4% at 2 standard frequencies, 250 and 500 hz (comparing rather favorably with normals) and suggesting that proper practise could greatly improve discrimination among vowels and voice-pitch journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 s. richard silverman changes. the point made here is that these features of auditory capacity need to be identified and subsequently cultivated. it emphasizes that we need to be cautious about regarding hearing in a simple two dimensional way. we look forward to making better use of improved description, if not always measurement, of all kinds of perceptual responsiveness and of intellectual, motor, emotional and social competence. granted the hopeful prospect that we shall be equipped with more useful information about young children we shall still face a number of alternatives for management that on first consideration are very appealing. consider, if you will, sensory stimulation and the options for channel, mode, and stimulus coding. here we are immediately confronted by the uni-multi-sensory issue. does a deliberate bisensory approach confuse and distract or does it reinforce with associated cues a primary channel of communication of another sense? and if it does either of these, how much, when and under what conditions? there is some evidence that tactual v o c o d e r s 1 2 , 1 7 or acoustic cues at the level of detectability, may help speechreading.8 the evidence for cooperation or competition between the auditory and visual senses is not all clear nor too abundantly at hand. we are not too much better off for convincing evidence for a bimodal unisensory tactic such as finger spelling combined with speechreading. some view these as the essential components of "total communication" to develop speech, speech reception and language, including reading.14 in the symposium last week, a number of systems cueing speech was discussed as an alternative bimodal approach. incidentally, they varied substantially in their primary objectives and in their relation to the utterances they are designed to cue. some are related to acoustic features, others to articulatory gestures, and still others to arbitrary differentiating cues for non-visible features. an impressive effort has gone into the coding of stimuli particularly auditory. the emphasis has been on making available crucial information-bearing elements of signals to the part'of the auditory channel that may possibly detect and transmit them most often in the low frequencies. clever methods of frequency transposition, insertion of discriminable surrogate signals and amplification of various sorts in association with particular training regimes are being attempted. here too the evidence for special methods of coding are not overwhelmingly convincing but impressionistic and anecdotal evaluation points to continued efforts in these directions. also we shall watch with interest the attempts, to adapt finger spelling to phbnetic correspondence and to expand and to refine syntactical features of the language of signs. / it becomes clearer as we ponder these alternatives for sensory input that we need to ask more fundamental questions such as the effects of a particular strategy on the operation of short term memory, the place of immediate, delayed, ambiguous or distorted feedback over any sensory system, and of the storage of rules of sequential probabilities of linguistic units. the task of improving our programs for young children is complicated further by the imposing and awesome array of theoretical alternatives and their application having to do with language acquisition. the ideas of piaget1 6 stressing that mental growth does not depend on quantitative addition of experience journal of thesouth african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) today and tomorrow in the education of the deaf 63 but that the development of thought and language is primarily qualitative has had substantial influence. thought precedes language and they are eventually interrelated. and even without language a child is capable of developing a logical-symbolic system. relevant to this we have said in hearing and deaf19 ness "there appears to be a significant movement to introduce the language of signs as a major mode of communication with young deaf children on the grounds that the capacity for thinking must be developed early and should not be confused with the capacity for using language. it is argued that we deter the development of thinking in deaf persons by emphasizing at the outset verbal means of communication, be they speech, the manual alphabet, or a combination of the two. in his book on the subject, furth9 indicates that by present methods we foster an 'experiential deficiency which would be avoidable if nonverbal methods of instruction and communication were encouraged both at home in the earliest years and in formal school education'. the assumption that this is the only alternative to certain conventional unproductive methods is open to question. for example, we must weigh carefully the accomplishment of parent guidance and the early intensive use of residual hearing, and also the impressive academic, vocational, and communicative attainment of many deaf persons." "the idea has been advanced that oral language be taught as a second language preceded by the language of signs aimed at cognitive development and that the latter be used to bring out the advantages of oral language. the sign language, it is suggested, ought to be enriched so that it is less rigidly concrete and situation-bound." "the 'oral-manual' controversy is not yet settled. it is encouraging, however, that numerous investigations are under way to study not only the linguistic, conceptual, and intellectual effects of modes of communication for deaf persons but also their influence on features of personality such as emotional maturity and self identity." skinner,20 as we know, emphasizes environmental conditioning and reinforcement. the child in discriminating the many stimuli in his environment emits sounds that get reinforced. the emphasis here in shaping verbal behavior is external and the child's internal structure is not of much importance. some of us believe that a primary cause of so called "oral failures" is the absence of a reinforcing environment for oral communication. skinner's views appear to be directly opposed to those of chomsky4 who postulates a theory of linguistic universals common to all cultures. chomsky asserts that a valid model of linguistic behavior must account for the extraordinary fact that we use language we have never heard before. very young children are able to construct and understand an impressive number of utterances that are quite new to them. from earliest childhood the human use of language goes far beyond what is "taught". therefore, there must be some fundamental process at work independent of input from the environment. in the absence of his ability to know what goes on in the mind the linguist imputes to it a finite set of rules that specifies the deep abstract grammatical structures that underlie sentences and then further transformational rules that relate these deep structures to tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 s. richard silverman surface structures (i.e. real sentences). the possibility of developing a better language program growing out of these ideas has become very intriguing to teachers of deaf children. the sequence of structured word strings in the language of normal children could be a model for teaching young deaf children. there would need to be contrived activities to do this for deaf children, not to mention a sorting out of "performance" and "competence" in our observations of a child's linguistic behavior. the reason for making the distinction is that it is in the description of linguistic competence that rules seem unavoidable as explanatory concepts.5in a meeting which we both attended, (princeton) i asked prof. chomsky, "how can we tap these (language) universale in creating a command of language in deaf children." i quote his answer: "one would expect that unless the appropriate stimulus conditions are realized, the instinctive behavior would not appear. it may be that the appropriate stimulus condition is hearing enough linguistic noises in your environment. so it just might be that there is no way to tap the system, any more than there is a way of initiating the system of flight in birds without putting them in the situation in which they have to flap their wings."6 note the phrase "appropriate stimulus condition". o'neil's15 word of caution about the application of psycholinguistcs to teaching is worthy of mention: linguistics thus seeks t o find explanations of the structures of language, for the relationships among structures, consistent explanations of the complicated and fragmentary data of language. it is further c o n c e r n e d t o offer explanations of t h e w a y in w h i c h t h e grammar is used and t h e w a y in w h i c h it is acquired and internalized b y infants growing up in s o c i e t y . t h e grammar d o e s not purport t o b e a m o d e l of h o w the human m i n d puts sentences together in speaking or takes t h e m apart in hearing. but t h e grammar d o e s presumably c o n s t i t u t e the k n o w l e d g e of his native language that the h u m a n b e i n g brings t o bear o n t h e tasks of speaking (and writing) and of hearing (and reading). very little of this is at all well u n d e r s t o o d , b u t there is m u c h interesting work in progress in psycholinguistics and s o m e tantalizing b i t s of information are emerging. much of the psycholinguistic research is entering areas of interest t o educators. but o n l y entering, n o t y e t there. it w o u l d b e a serious mistake t o begin building educational programs in areas where our understanding is so dim. in fact w e have nothing like a c o m p l e t e l y formulated grammar of any language, m u c h less a c o m p l e t e understanding of language in general. of interest to us, of course, is vygotsky's21 notion of "inner speech." it does suggest that we must develop ways, as i have said elsewhere, of rummaging around in the psyche of deaf children to study the relation of the symbols they use for expressing themselves be they oral, finger spelling or signs and their thought processes. this need is underlined by lennenberg's13 views that language behavior derives from peculiarly human cognitive processes like categorization and abstraction. it is a human mode of analysing experiences into conceptual units and rules that link them. the. child matures in this process biologically but language stimulation is necessary in his formative years. bronowski and bellugi2 tell us that "most of what we regard as objects in our environment, however, are sophisticated concepts. thus the logic by which a child unravels the sentences he hears and his experience of the environment together is much more than a capacity for language and expresses in miniature journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) today and tomorrow in the education of the deaf 65 a deeper human capacity for analyzing and manipulating the environment in the mind by subdividing it into units that persist when they are moved from one mental context into another." so even learning the name of an object by a deaf child is a much more cognitive act than heretofore supposed and should influence our methods accordingly. in hearing and deafness18 we have said, it is appropriate, in discussing the young deaf child, to mention the increasing amount of information and guidance available for parents of deaf children. although here and there an effort may be misguided, the proliferation of parent institutes and clinics, and of correspondence courses, reading lists, and literary output is one of the most constructive and forward-looking developments in the education of the deaf. one parent put it succinctly: . . . the tough thing about deafness is likely to be social isolation, social adjustment. there is no one in the world, and there never can be anyone, as important in determining any child's social adjustment as that child's own parents and his family at home. for that reason parents are important. in general there appear to be no universally accepted specific aims or procedures in guiding parents of very young children. the emphases vary. for some, the primary aim is to create realistic "acceptance" of the child's condition, and counseling is weighted toward psychotherapy. for others, the emphasis is on conveying information in order to create an understanding of sensory deprivation and its effect on the total development of the child in general and of his communication deficit in particular. whenever possible there is a growing trend toward carrying on parent "training" in homes and homelike settings, sometimes called demonstration homes, where, by demonstration and practice, parents learn to contrive and take advantage of natural situations in the home to sharpen perceptions and foster communication. of great interest to educators of deaf children is the knowledge likely to be gained from the programs of early education such as head start directed at "culturally disadvantaged" children3 in the u.s.a., in the u.s.s.r.1, and in the kibbutzim of israel. here too, vigorous schools of thought appear to be taking shape. on the one hand, there are those who would emphasize "cognitive" approaches that stimulate intellectual functioning. in its extreme form it has been labelled the "pressure-cooker" view, which aims to compensate for the lack of opportunity for perceptual development. others would stress the child's social and emotional growth without too much "structured" teaching. here, too, we need a better understanding of the role of shaped sensory experience in the cognitive development of infants.11 such evidence as exists for the value of particular procedures and programs of parent counseling is meager and is generally anecdotal or based on studies (frequently retroactive) of children's records. it will be helpful, for the parent programs we undertake, to evaluate all of the following: genetic counseling for deaf married couples and parents of deaf children; the use of the high risk register; the adaptations necessary because of differences in the intelligence, motivation, and education of parents; the emotional needs of parents; the special training necessary for those who counsel parents; and the implications of the concept of critical periods in development. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 s. richard silverman we have seen in these necessarily selective and illustrative remarks that the rising tide of proposed approaches is swamping teachers with more untested devices and strategies than they are prepared to absorb or evaluate. we also need to note that our investigators may be properly criticised for violating any number of principles of scientific inquiry having to do with accurate description of methods being compared, with recognition of individual differences among deaf children, with statistical treatment, with poor measuring instruments, with asking wrong questions, and with premature judgments that do not wait on longitudinal effects to appear. yet in the today and tomorrow of our field we must look to constructive collaboration of investigators and practitioners to the end that we may choose our alternatives on a rational basis. references 1. bronfenbrenner, u. (1970): two worlds of childhood. u.s. and u.s.sr. basic books, new york. 2. bronowski, j. and bellugi, u. (1970): language, name and concept. science, 168,669-673. 3. caldwell, b.m. (1970): the rationale for early intervention. exceptional children, 36, 717-726. 4. chomsky, n. (1964): current issues in linguistic theory. mouton, the hague. 5. chomsky, n. (1965): aspects of the theory of syntax. the m.i.t. press, cambridge, mass. 6. chomsky, n. (1967): general properties of language. in brain mechanisms underlying speech and language (page 73), darley, f.l. ed., grune and stratton, new york. 7. davis, h. (1970): chapter 4, in hearing and deafness, davis, h. and silverman, s.r. (eds.), holt, rinehart and winston, new york. 8. erber, n.p. (1970): auditory and audio-visual reception of words in noise by observers with normal and impaired hearing, unpublished ph.d. thesis, washington university, st. louis, missouri. 9. furth, h.g. (1966): thinking without language: psychologicalimpli. cations of deafness. the free press, new york. 10. gengel, r.w. (1969): practice effects in frequency discrimination by hearing impaired children. j. of speech and hearing research, 12, 847-855. 11. kagan, j. (1970): the determinants of attention in the infant. american scientist, 3,298-305. 12. kringlebotn, m. (1968): experiments with some visual and vibrotactile aids for the deaf. a merican a nnals of the deaf, 113,311-317. 13. lenneberg, e.h. (1967): biological foundations of language. m.i.t. press, cambridge, mass. 14. moores, d.f. (1970): psycholinguistics and deafness.american annals of the deaf, 115,37-48. 15. o'neil, w.a. (1968):'paul roberts' rules of order: the misuses of linguistics in the classroom. the urban review, 2,12-16. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) today and tomorrow in the education of the deaf 67 16. piaget, j. (1954): the construction of reality in the child. translated by cook, m., basic books, inc., new york. 17. pickett, j.m. and pickett, r.h. (1970): communication of speech sounds by a tactual vocoder. j. of speech and hearing research, 6, 202-222. 18. silverman, s.r. (1964): education of deaf children past and prologue. in report of the proceedings of the international congress on the education of the deaf and the forty-first meeting of the convention of american instructors of the deaf, u.s. document no. 106, u.s. government printing office, washington, d.c., 113-122. 19. silverman, s.r. and lane, h.s. (1970): chapter \6, hearing and deafness, davis, h. and silverman, s.r. (eds.). holt, rinehart and winston, new york. 20. skinner, b.f. (1957): verbal behavior. appleton-century-crofts, inc., new york. 21. vygotsky, l. (1962): thought and language. m.i.t. press, cambridge, mass. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abstract introduction method participants materials procedures data analysis results discussion development, faceand content validity of the neonatal feeding assessment scale outcome of the delphi process conclusion acknowledgements references about the author(s) mari viviers department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa alta kritzinger department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa bart vinck department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa citation viviers, m., kritzinger, a., & vinck, b. (2016). development of a clinical feeding assessment scale for very young infants in south africa. south african journal of communication disorders 63(1), a148. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i1.148 original research development of a clinical feeding assessment scale for very young infants in south africa mari viviers, alta kritzinger, bart vinck received: 08 jan. 2016; accepted: 07 aug. 2016; published: 26 oct. 2016 copyright: © 2016. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract background: there is a need for validated neonatal feeding assessment instruments in south africa. a locally developed instrument may contribute to standardised evaluation procedures of high-risk neonates and address needs in resource constrained developing settings. objective: the aim of the study was to develop and validate the content of a clinical feeding assessment scale to diagnose oropharyngeal dysphagia (opd) in neonates. method: the neonatal feeding assessment scale (nfas) was developed using the delphi method. five international and south african speech-language therapists (slts) formed the expert panel, participating in two rounds of electronic questionnaires to develop and validate the content of the nfas. results: all participants agreed on the need for the development of a valid clinical feeding assessment instrument to use with the neonatal population. the initial nfas consisted of 240 items across 8 sections, and after the delphi process was implemented, the final format was reduced to 211 items across 6 sections. the final format of the nfas is scored using a binary scoring system guiding the clinician to diagnose the presence or absence of opd. all members agreed on the format, the scoring system and the feeding constructs addressed in the revised final format of the nfas. conclusion: the delphi method and the diverse clinical and research experience of participants could be integrated to develop the nfas which may be used in clinical practice in south africa or similar developing contexts. because of demographically different work settings marked by developed versus developing contexts, participants did not have the same expectations of a clinical dysphagia assessment. the international participants contributed to evidence-based content development. local participants considered the contextual challenges of south african slts entering the field with basic competencies in neonatal dysphagia management, thereby justifying a comprehensive clinical instrument. the nfas is aimed at clinicians working in neonatal intensive care units where they manage large caseloads of high-risk neonates. further validation of the nfas is recommended to determine its criterion validity in comparison with a widely accepted standard such as the modified barium swallow study. introduction clinical assessment is an important part of evidence-based management of neonatal dysphagia (thoyre, park, pados & hubbard, 2013). the purpose of clinical assessment is to establish the possible nature of the feeding problem, to explore the parent’s perception of the problem and the neonate’s readiness for oral feeding, to make a differential diagnosis and to determine the need for multi-disciplinary management (arvedson, 2008; rommel, 2006; thoyre et al., 2013). the two main components of such an assessment include a parent interview and medical chart review – to obtain the feeding, medical and developmental history – as well as the clinical feeding assessment (arvedson, 2008; lau & smith, 2011). with the development of a novel clinical assessment instrument, the researchers acknowledge the importance of comprehensive clinical assessment, but concurs with studies (arvedson, 2008; de matteo, matovich & hjartarson, 2005; rommel, 2006) that clinical assessment is not designed to replace objective instrumental assessment such as the modified barium swallow study (mbss). a clinical instrument should support an accurate diagnosis and description of the feeding profile related to oropharyngeal dysphagia (opd) in high-risk neonates. the use of validated instruments should be encouraged in clinical practice because it provides a common language among clinicians, facilitates the production of diagnostic data and promotes the evaluation of techniques and approaches used during clinical assessment (brandao, dos santos & lanzilotti, 2013). there is a high prevalence of low birth weight (lbw) and prematurity in south africa (who, 2012) contributing to neonatal opd. in the usa, the prevalence of feeding disorders in premature neonates is estimated between 10.5% and 24.5% (jadcherla, 2016). currently, no prevalence figures on feeding disorders associated with prematurity are available in south africa. the high prevalence of feeding disorders among the neonatal population supports the need for appropriate early clinical assessment and management of opd, providing an impetus for the development of a valid clinical instrument to contribute to differential diagnosis. in the south african public healthcare sector, there are resource constraints such as limited or no speech-language therapists (slts) to provide feeding services in some neonatal intensive care units (nicus) (strasheim, kritzinger & louw, 2011). slts working in hospitals are also required to manage large caseloads apart from neonatal dysphagia and then do not have the opportunity to specialise in the field. in addition, inexperienced community service therapists are frequently the only service providers in some settings (singh et al., 2015). existing dysphagia assessment instruments may not meet the needs in south africa. philbin and ross (2011) developed the ‘support of oral feeding for fragile infants’ (soffi) which includes a systematic approach to assessment of bottle feeding and clinical decision-making for intervention. the department of health in south africa promotes the world health organization guidelines (who, 2010) for infant feeding which recommend exclusive breastfeeding for the first 6 months of life (national department of health, 2015). the bottle-feeding approach of the soffi therefore has limited application in the healthcare sector in south africa. some reliable clinical instruments that are supported by high-level evidence do exist, but do not focus holistically on neonatal feeding. the neonatal oral motor assessment schema [nomas] (palmer, crawley & blanco, 1992) and the schedule for oral motor assessment [soma] (reilly, skuse & wolke, 2000) both focus on oral motor skills only (pressman, 2010; rogers & arvedson, 2005). these two scales do not address a feeding assessment from a bio-psychosocial perspective to diagnose opd. such a perspective acknowledges the impact of nicu environmental stressors on state regulation, internal physiological disruptions on the neonate’s subsystems and the resulting effects on the feeding process, as well as mother–infant interaction during feeding. a clinical assessment instrument should assist the slt to assess all neonatal systems that contribute to and interact with the feeding process. the instrument should consider the sequential development of the sensory systems emerging throughout gestation in a developmentally supportive approach (browne & ross, 2011; thoyre, 2007). such an instrument should also be comprehensive to facilitate the description of symptoms related to sensory and motor-based feeding difficulties (lau & smith, 2011) that may result in opd from 32 weeks gestational age. neonatal opd is any interference with the acts of feeding and/or swallowing that interrupts the oral or pharyngeal stage of swallowing compromising the development of typical feeding and swallowing skills and the neonate’s nutritional and respiratory status (arvedson, 2008; browne & ross, 2011; rogers & arvedson, 2005). the condition is typically only diagnosed from 32 weeks gestational age when nutritive sucking (ns) should emerge (rogers & arvedson, 2005; thoyre, 2007). to facilitate the assessment process, an instrument should provide prompts for observation of a variety of signs and symptoms related to neonatal opd. the purpose of neonatal feeding assessment is to accurately diagnose opd to prevent the negative sequalae of opd. such negative effects may include inadequate weight gain, dehydration, and limited oral sensory experience, which may continue to impact on infancy and early childhood. obtaining expert opinions on such a new instrument would be invaluable for the development and validation process. this article will report on experts’ opinion on the development of the content and face validity of a clinical feeding assessment instrument. method aims the aim was to develop and validate the content of a novel clinical feeding assessment scale to diagnose opd in neonates. the objectives to support the aim were (1) to determine if the panel of experts agreed about the need for a validated clinical feeding assessment scale, (2) to select appropriate items for inclusion in the neonatal feeding assessment scale (nfas) and lastly (3) to establish face and content validity of the nfas based on expert input. design the delphi method (hassan, keeney & mckenna, 2000) was used to gather quantitative and qualitative data from an expert panel during two rounds of consecutive questionnaires. qualitative data were obtained from open questions, and quantitative data from closed questions. the delphi method was used to guide improvement of content and face validity of the new instrument. this method allowed the researchers to investigate whether the nfas represented all facets of neonatal feeding skills. the primary strength of the delphi method is the objective exploration of issues that require judgement, such as the content and measurement methods when developing a clinical assessment instrument. because the delphi method is considered one of the most commonly used research procedures to establish content validity of an assessment instrument by an expert panel (hassan et al., 2000), this design was considered suitable for the purpose of this study. participants five expert panel members were included in the study. informed consent was obtained from all participants. participant selection criteria included a masters’ degree qualification in speech-language pathology from an accredited tertiary institution to guarantee a high level of expertise and at least 5 years clinical experience in the field of paediatric dysphagia. participants could reside in south africa or internationally. in table 1, a summary of participant characteristics is provided. table 1: participant description (n = 5). all participants had postgraduate qualifications in the field of speech-language pathology. both international experts had doctoral degrees in paediatric dysphagia which demonstrated their advanced knowledge. in addition, the international experts had more than 20 years of clinical experience working in paediatric dysphagia. this highlighted the long history of paediatric dysphagia intervention in the usa as well as the experts’ significant clinical experience. only one of the south african participants had more than 20 years’ clinical experience. materials the nfas will not be described in detail in this section because the purpose of the study was to develop and validate the content of the instrument. the nfas was based on other clinical assessment instruments, studies on neonatal feeding development, relevant literature on prematurity, lbw and paediatric hiv and/or aids in the south african context and recent studies on neonatal dysphagia. additionally, the first author’s clinical experience of service delivery in the private and public healthcare sectors in the nicu provided insight into local needs and knowledge of specific local constraints. two self-composed electronic questionnaires were used to obtain feedback from the expert panel on the content of the nfas. round one required a comprehensive overview of the nfas and round two required targeted responses in closed question format about the revised content, structure and format of the nfas. the two questionnaires contained questions on the relevance of separate sections and items relating to the different neonatal systems involved in feeding in the nfas. both questionnaires gave the participants the opportunity to offer recommendations on the addition or removal of sections and items, to comment on different scoring methods, and to judge the comprehensiveness of the scale and its relevance to clinical use in hospitals. open-ended and some close-ended questions were also included addressing face validity, user friendliness, and the format of the instrument and technical editing (dawson & trapp, 2004). for close-ended questions, reasons for answers had to be given. the questionnaires facilitated a deductive reasoning sequence to compile an authentic profile of neonatal feeding skill assessment. the first questionnaire focused on the content domains of skills related to neonatal feeding and swallowing (als et al., 1994; arvedson, 2008; arvedson & brodsky, 2002; bahr, 2001; brazelton, 1973; browne & ross, 2011; clark, 2009; da costa & van der schans, 2008; darrow & harley, 1998; dieckmann, brownstein & gausche-hill, 2006; gewolb & vice, 2006; hall, 2001, 2011; henning, 2002; hodgman, hoppenbrouwers & cabal, 1993; jadcherla, 2016; karl, 2004; nugent, 2007; prechtl & beintema, 1964; qureshi, vice, taciak, bosma & gewolb, 2002; rudolph & link, 2002; swigert, 2010; tsai, chen & lin, 2010; van haastert, de vries, helders & jongmans, 2006; wolff, 1959; wolf & glass, 1992) – see table 2. a draft version of the nfas accompanied the first questionnaire. table 2: content and rationale for expert panel questionnaire 1. the second questionnaire was developed based on the responses and feedback obtained in the first questionnaire. the nfas was adapted according to the experts’ feedback. the revised nfas was then sent to the expert panel along with the summary of changes recommended in the first round. the second questionnaire was used to further refine the content and face validity of the instrument (table 3). table 3: content and rationale for expert panel questionnaire 2. procedures clearance was obtained from the research ethics committee at the university where the study was conducted. the process of validation of a new assessment instrument commences with the initial development phase providing a sound theoretical foundation to link to clinical practice (st pierre et al., 2010). the initial phase of instrument development consisted of the review of available published scales, checklists and literature, and the researchers’ own clinical experience. the second phase employed the delphi method to request expert judgement on the new clinical instrument. the panel members’ identity was blinded to one another to enhance open participation in the instrument development process. the procedures followed in the study are depicted in figure 1. figure 1: flowchart of study procedures. the preliminary and revised instrument was sent to the expert panel to facilitate two rounds of questioning via email. the panel was blind to one another’s responses. the aim of the first round was to allow the expert panel to judge the validity of the content domains in the instrument. summarised feedback, to the panel, after round one served as the introduction of round two. the aim of the second round was to reach consensus on the recommendations of the first round, as well as on the content and the scoring system of the instrument. after the second round responses were received from the participants, the delphi process was concluded, as majority agreement and no new additional content was suggested, indicating that adequate consensus among panel members had been reached. the delphi method allowed rich data to be gathered because open and closed questions could be used to probe the participants’ views on the nfas. round one rendered descriptive data which was analysed according to emerging themes linked to the various content sections of the draft instrument. data analysis according to hassan et al. (2000), the delphi method is not intended to produce statistically significant results, but rather a synthesis of an expert group’s opinion. suggested changes according to the themes that emerged from the data will be discussed. sections of the data of round one and all of the data from round two were analysed quantitatively using frequency counts. results results will be presented according to the three objectives of the study. objective a determining agreement on the need for a validated feeding assessment instrument three themes were identified linked to the content sections of the first questionnaire. the first content theme was the need for a valid assessment tool. the second theme was content of the nfas and the last was scoring criteria. only the first theme’s results are discussed with this objective. questions 4 and 5 in the first questionnaire investigated the rationale for the development of the nfas. all participants (n = 5; 100%) agreed that the development of a valid clinical assessment tool was a relevant area of study and confirmed the need for such a tool. some participants also provided further comments to reflect their agreement. it was stated that: ‘…there is definitely a need for a well-researched assessment tool for use with infants…’ [participant 4, female, slt] however: ‘internationally still a huge lack of normative data regarding sucking and swallowing along with more global developmental aspects of feeding in young infants….difficulty lies in subjectivity of observation of skills that are not measurable…’ [participant 2, female, slt] one of the south african panel members commented that: ‘…in south african public healthcare an instrument would help with prioritisation of a large case load on assessment outcomes that are valid…and prevent over referral to video swallows….’ [participant 5, female, slt] in addition, one of the participants stated that a validated feeding assessment instrument might support clinicians in case management. the qualitative comments further supported the rationale for research to develop a validated feeding assessment instrument for use with the neonatal population. objective b content and item selection of the neonatal feeding assessment scale the content and item selection of the preliminary nfas was based on theoretical constructs related to neonatal feeding and the clinical assessment of feeding difficulty in early infancy. the instrument relies on physiological observations of the neonate during feeding and elicitation of oral responses. neonatal states were included so that the influence on feeding and state disruption as a result of feeding difficulty may be observed. the structure of the initial draft of the nfas included three different age categories – from 32 weeks gestational age to 4 months corrected age post term. these different age categories allowed for the inclusion of developmentally appropriate items. in table 4, the content of the nfas and the rationale for content selection are summarised. table 4: preliminary neonatal feeding assessment scale content and rationale for item selection. all five participants contributed to both rounds of the delphi process resulting in a 100% response rate. the results of rounds one and two are presented separately. the thematic analysis of the first theme of round one was discussed, and examples of panel member responses to complement the data were provided in the first section of the results; however, in this section results related to the second and third themes are presented. the closed question responses of rounds one and two are combined and will be presented in table format. results of round one the second theme addressed the content of the nfas, it was stated: ‘…it is a very comprehensive tool covering all necessary areas…’. [participant 1, female, slt] a similar comment was made by participant 2. however, it was stated that: ‘….section g [parent-neonate interaction] and h [use of compensatory strategies] are not that relevant to first-time assessment…i view it as part of treatment already…consider removing it from the current instrument’. [participant 5, female, slt] three of the participants indicated that these two subsections were too subjective and not directly relevant to initial assessment and diagnosis of opd. these subsections were then omitted from the final instrument. four participants also suggested revision of some of the items related to feeding and swallowing ability in the content domains in sections c, e and f. based on some participant’s feedback (n = 3), there was support for the notion of a comprehensive clinical assessment in the neonatal stage, despite indicating that the instrument was too lengthy. the recommended scoring system of the nfas (theme three) included allocation of marks if a skill and/or behaviour was present or absent. the clinician would then calculate a score for each section and a final score for feeding difficulties to conclude the assessment. the higher the score, the more likely a neonate could be diagnosed with opd. theme three dealt with the scoring criteria. statements were made, such as: ‘…consider simplifying the scoring system for ease of use…might be confusing in current format’ and ‘[y]ou need to score a concept to compare it to a gold standard to be able to validate it’ [participant 1, female, slt] another comment was: ‘…the scoring system will be easier if binary scoring in a checklist format is used in the final version of the instrument….with a good explanation of administration guidelines…’ [participant 4, female, slt] one of the south african participants stated: ‘the scoring system is a bit confusing in this format…instructions on how to assess the neonate should be expanded…since some speech therapists might lack experience….and need help…’ [participant 5, female, slt] three participants suggested clearer administration guidelines and using a different approach to score the data. results of round one led to the refinement of the initial scoring system. binary choices were included for each item in all sections, with clear administration and scoring guidelines in the revised instrument. the scoring method was refined with assistance from a biostatistician to include a binary (yes/no) outcome for each section and a total score that will enable comparison with a widely accepted gold standard for swallowing assessment, in this case the mbss. in summary, all participants agreed on the need for more research to develop a validated assessment instrument. three of the five participants agreed on the comprehensive nature of the proposed content for the draft nfas. lastly, all the participants recommended refinement of the scoring system. however, differences in opinion encountered in the feedback from participants in round one were analysed further to highlight how the south african panel members’ responses differed from the international participants’ contributions. these differences may be as a result of the disparity of resources between the developing and developed context of the participants, and challenges experienced in the local context that international participants may not be aware of. a difference in opinion was clearly evident between the two groups of panel members about the length of the instrument and item inclusion of which both components related to the comprehensive nature of the nfas. results of round two upon conclusion of round one, the nfas was revised according to recommended changes where the majority opinion (dawson & trapp, 2004) motivated the changes. to initiate round two, a summary of the first round’s recommendations and the revised instrument were sent to the participants. objective c face and content validity of the final version of the nfas the second questionnaire provided quantitative data that could be compared with some of the close-ended questions in round one. round two offered an opportunity for additional comments by the panel members if they felt that the previous round did not address all their concerns. the comparative results of the two rounds are depicted in table 5. table 5: quantification of degree of agreement among participants. according to table 5, the majority of panel members’ (n = 4) opinions regarding some of the concepts probed in round one and again in round two (closed questions) reflected increased agreement on the probed components of the final version of the nfas. one participant did not agree on the user friendliness, content and face validity in round one. to ensure scientific rigor, the delphi process holds researchers accountable by providing a true account of the participation responses. as participants responded via email, data could be saved and verified. no qualitative comments were received in round two. according to table 4, there were a number of disagreements in round one that was resolved in round two, which indicated high agreement among the panel. all members agreed on the format, the scoring system and the feeding constructs addressed in the revised final format of the nfas. the final content and checklist format of the nfas, which resulted from the delphi process, consisted of six sections with different items. the nfas is summarised in table 6. table 6: overview of the final neonatal feeding assessment scale. all the changes were made based on majority recommendations of the expert panel. according to table 6, one subsection in section a contained nine items relating to the discolouration of the neonate’s skin indicating lack of oxygen in the orofacial area. the majority of participants considered these items too subjective for accurate scoring, and therefore, it was removed. section b remained unchanged because participants suggested no changes. in section c, eight items relating to various stress cues were removed because of possible ambiguity, repetitiveness or vagueness indicated by three participants. section d was reduced from 17 to 12 items to screen muscle tone and movement in a more concise manner because five of the items were considered redundant by four participants. in the last two sections (e and f), items suggested by all the participants were added to ensure comprehensive observations of oral structure as well as neonatal feeding and swallowing skills. however, the international panel members recommended that subsections (in sections e and f) relating to physical symptoms of illness (e.g. oral thrush in neonates with hiv and/or aids), saliva management and feeding methods should rather be obtained from the neonate’s medical record or during the parent interview, and therefore, it was removed. in some of the subgroupings in sections e and f, where feeding skills relate to developmental level, two age categories were linked to assessment items and criteria leading to a reorganisation of items. all the participants agreed on the use of these age categories. age categories may enable serial assessment to build a feeding profile over time whilst the neonate is receiving hospital-based care. the components of comprehensive clinical feeding assessment that emerged were the observation of physiological status, state of alertness, stress cues, postural control and tone related to feeding position, oral-motor structure and function, non-nutritive sucking (nns) and ns, behavioural responses to feeding and symptoms of opd (dodrill, cleghorn, donovan & davies, 2008; lau & smith, 2011; thoyre et al., 2013). these components were all addressed in the revised nfas. the length of the instrument relates to the local need and aim of a comprehensive assessment tool which should include signs and symptoms reflecting the presence of opd in neonates. discussion need of the neonatal feeding assessment scale the need of a clinical tool to assess opd in high-risk neonates was established. in a review of oral feeding assessment instruments for infants younger than 6 months, the findings of pados et al. (2016) support the identification of this need. they concluded that there is a need for the development and testing of feeding assessment tools for young infants to guide optimal clinical practice. it is also suggested that such assessment tools should allow use for breast and bottle feeding for consistent assessment across feeding methods. meeting this need may facilitate more appropriate management of opd in neonates, because intervention will be guided by reliable and comprehensive assessment findings with an accompanying diagnosis. infants discharged with inadequate investigation into the feeding difficulties or unresolved feeding difficulties, lbw and prematurity are more at risk of developing failure-to-thrive than their term counterparts with appropriate weight for age (browne & ross, 2011). valid and reliable assessment instruments will help clinicians to objectively evaluate feeding (pados et al., 2016). development, faceand content validity of the neonatal feeding assessment scale the delphi method was used to develop the final format of the nfas and to establish face and content validity. this was achieved by convening an expert panel to assist with the further development of a novel clinical feeding assessment instrument. the interaction process was collaborative and yielded constructive comments supporting the validation of the nfas. the delphi process was helpful to consider appropriate feeding constructs for content selection, to develop a reliable scoring system and to enable transparency and replication of methodology. differences in opinion between the local and international participants emerged and may likely be ascribed to the working context in developing versus developed countries, emphasising the challenges present in the south african context. the participants’ comments supported the rationale of the study regarding the development of a neonatal feeding assessment instrument supported by evidence, but also highlighted the subjective nature of observation of skills related to neonatal feeding. this calls for more research on objective measurement of skills related to feeding difficulties in neonates. the south african participants did not see a need to shorten the nfas significantly because they felt that it ensures holistic and comprehensive clinical assessment that might be lacking in inexperienced clinicians. in contrast, the international participants were of the opinion that the instrument was too lengthy for clinical use in the initial version. this may be due to the international experts being more experienced than some of the south african participants in clinical practice, because both of the international experts had more than 20 years’ experience working in the field of paediatric dysphagia. the participant responses assisted the researchers in refining the content and items of the nfas. south african participants considered comprehensiveness as important in clinical service delivery in resource constrained settings. many inexperienced clinicians are conducting their community service year and require guidance. a comprehensive assessment instrument may prompt observations which may be missed when item descriptions are omitted. international participants focused on the subjectivity of some items which revealed that they were more experienced and therefore concerned with the levels of evidence to support the inclusion of sections and items, especially in a context where inexperienced slts may be using the nfas. no difference in opinion regarding the scoring criteria and guidelines was noted. however, one international participant was the only expert who recommended consultation with a biostatistician, demonstrating knowledge of instrument development acquired during her research career. owing to demographically different work settings marked by developed versus developing contexts impacting on healthcare service delivery, participants did not have the same expectations of a clinical assessment. the local participants were aware of inexperienced slts entering the public health system in their community service year and having to diagnose opd without mbss equipment. the nfas was designed to prompt inexperienced slts to include appropriate content domains during clinical assessment and supports a comprehensive approach to assessment of neonatal feeding problems such as opd. paediatric and adult dysphagia were formally included as a module in undergraduate speech-language pathology curricula in 2004 in south africa (see faculty of humanities undergraduate syllabi and regulations, 2004, university of pretoria as an example). there is thus only an 11-year history of formal professional training at universities in south africa. although dysphagia is now an established component of local speech-language pathology curricula, much research is still required. dysphagia is a relatively new, yet growing field in the profession in south africa with active pursuit of research (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010; pike, pike, kritzinger, krüger & viviers, 2016; singh et al., 2015). outcome of the delphi process the participants had the opportunity to critically evaluate the revised nfas as indicated by their change in responses in round two, leading to majority consensus (see table 3). one of the members who did not agree on the user friendliness of the draft instrument still indicated that the nfas was too lengthy despite revision. this concern already emerged in round one and was addressed through implementing the recommended changes (see table 6) and using a checklist format that improved effectiveness. the same participant indicated that the face and content validity were not completely adequate because many observations remained subjective in nature. the researchers attempted to include measurable items where possible to decrease subjectivity; however, this was not possible for all items. there remains a great need for further research on neonatal feeding skills and objective measurement technologies. the validity of content and items were supported by using current research on developmental skills and feeding abilities of neonates. the aforementioned concerns were addressed as far as possible in the final format of the nfas. when interpreting results in a delphi process, the majority opinion motivated the changes, but if a valid contribution is offered by a single participant or a minority, the researchers may choose to use it (okoli & pawlowski, 2004). in the revised nfas, local needs were paramount and the south african participants preferred a comprehensive assessment instrument. similar to the nfas, other researchers in health sciences also found the delphi method useful in contributing to the successful development of clinically relevant assessment instruments (crist, dobbelsteyn, brousseau & napier-phillips, 2004; da costa, van den engelhoek & bos, 2008; schulz et al., 2009; yousuf, 2007). the nfas is aimed at clinicians working in nicus, where they manage large caseloads of very young high-risk populations. an increased prevalence of high-risk neonates exists in developing countries such as south africa (who, 2012). early identification of opd whilst these neonates are still accessible in the hospital is important to allow opportunity to train mothers to manage feeding difficulties before discharge. in addition, opd appears to be more prevalent than growth problems in preterm neonates and is likely to continue into early childhood, thereby indicating the need for early intervention to address feeding difficulties and minimise caregiver stress (crist et al., 2004). the nfas aims to provide a developmentally supportive approach to assessment as proposed by thoyre et al., (2013). the nfas is minimally invasive because assessment is mainly through observation of a broad scope of skills before and during feeding to prevent overloading neonatal sensory systems with physical handling. studies by philbin and ross (2011) as well as browne and ross (2011) indicated that unnecessary physical handling may disrupt state regulation during this sensitive stage of neurological development. another characteristic of the nfas includes the parent/caregiver in family-centred service delivery. mothers contribute greatly to feeding assessment by providing information about their infant, and their experience and feelings surrounding the feeding challenges. a family-centred developmentally supportive approach relates to current evidence in the field of neonatal dysphagia (lau & smith, 2011; thoyre et al., 2013). conclusion in south africa, the field of paediatric dysphagia was formally introduced to curricula at universities in 2004, but was practiced many years prior to this introduction. issues such as resource constraints, inadequate infrastructure, new graduates required to manage large caseloads in the public health system, few expert clinicians in practice and feeding difficulties related to hiv and/or aids are some of the challenges faced in practice (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010; singh et al., 2015). inexperienced clinicians may benefit from structured guidance provided by the nfas in a resource-restrained context where patient prioritisation is key. the inherent limitations of the delphi method include judgements of a select panel which may not be representative of the opinions of all clinicians. the time-consuming nature of participation which may impact on the thoroughness of the panel members’ responses, may also be a limitation. the final version of the nfas reflects relevant areas of neonatal feeding prominently. the item selection clearly indicates the wide array of skills and components forming the foundation of neonatal feeding behaviour and responses that should be included in a comprehensive assessment instrument to be used by slts. the final content and checklist format of the nfas was compiled as the first step in validating the nfas and will be used in a future study to determine the preliminary psychometric properties of this instrument. acknowledgements competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions m.v. was the project leader, collected data and analysed data. m.v. and a.k. prepared the manuscript. b.v. contributed to initial project design. references als, h. (1982). toward a synactive theory of development: promise for the assessment and support of infant individuality. infant mental health journal, 3, 229–243. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/1097-0355(198224)3:4%3c229::aid-imhj2280030405%3e3.0.co;2-h als, h., lawhon, g., duffy, f.h., mcanulty, g.b., gibes-grossman, r., & blickman, j.g. (1994). individualized developmental care for the very low birth-weight preterm infant. medical and neurofunctional effects. jama, 272, 853–858. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/jama.1994.03520110033025 arvedson, j.c. (2008). assessment of pediatric dysphagia and feeding disorders: clinical and instrumental approaches. developmental disabilities research reviews, 14, 118–127. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/ddrr.17 arvedson, j.c., & brodsky, l. (2002). pediatric swallowing and feeding: assessment and management. clifton park: thomson delmar learning. bahr, d.c. (2001). oral motor assessment and treatment: ages and stages. boston, ma: allyn and bacon. blackwell, z., & littlejohns, p. (2010). a review of the management of dysphagia: a south african perspective. journal of neuroscience nursing, 42(2), 61–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/jnn.0b013e3181ce5c86 brandao, e., dos santos, i., & lanzillotti, r.s. (2013). validation of an instrument to assess patients with skin conditions. acta paula enfermage, 26(5), 460–466. brazelton, t.b. (1973). neonatal behavioural assessment scale. clinics in developmental medicine no. 50. philadelphia, pa: j.p. lippincott. browne, j.v., & ross, e.s. (2011). eating as a neurodevelopmental process for high-risk newborns. clinical perinatology, 38, 731–743. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2011.08.004 clark, d.a. (2009). evaluation and care of the neonate. in g.l. ensher, d.a. clark, & n.s. songer (eds.), families, infants and young children at risk: pathways to best practice (pp. 29–38). baltimore, md: paul h brookes publishing co. crist, w., dobbelsteyn, c., brousseau, a.m., & napier-phillips, a. (2004). pediatric assessment scale for severe feeding problems: validity and reliability of a new scale for tube-fed children. nutrition in clinical practice, 19, 403–408. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0115426504019004403 da costa, s.p., van den engel-hoek, l., & bos, a.f. (2008). sucking and swallowing in infants and diagnostic tools. journal of perinatology, 28, 247–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.jp.7211924 da costa, s.p., & van der schans, c. (2008). the reliability of the neonatal oral-motor assessment scale. acta paediatrica, 97, 21–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00577.x darrow, d.h., & harley, c.m. (1998). evaluation of swallowing disorders in children. otolaryngology clinics of north america, 31(3), 405–418. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0030-6665(05)70061-xm dawson, b., & trapp, r.g. (2004). basic and clinical biostatistics. (4th edn.). new york: lange medical books/mcgraw-hill. de matteo, c., matovich, d., & hjartarson, a. (2005). comparison of clinical and videofluoroscopic evaluation of children with feeding and swallowing difficulties. developmental medicine & child neurology, 47(3), 149–157. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0012162205000289 dieckman, r., brownstein, d., & gausche-hill, m. (2000). american academy of paediatrics: pediatric education for pre-hospital professionals. boston, ma: jones & bartlett publishers. dodrill, p., cleghorn, g., donovan, t., & davies, p. (2008). growth patterns in preterm infants born appropriate for gestational age. journal of paediatric and child health, 44(6), 332–337. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1440-1754.2007.01269.x du plessis, e., & human, s.p. (2007). the art of the delphi technique: highlighting its scientific merit. health sa gesondheid, 12(4), 13–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/hsag.v12i4.268 faculty of humanities. (2004). undergraduate syllabi and regulations. university of pretoria, pretoria. gewolb, i.h., & vice, f.l. (2006). maturational changes in the rhythms, patterning, and coordination of respiration and swallow during feeding in preterm and term neonatals. developmental medicine & child neurology, 48, 589–594. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s001216220600123x hall, k.d. (2001). pediatric dysphagia: resource guide. san diego, ca: singular thomson learning. hassan, f., keeney, s., & mckenna, h. (2000). research guidelines for the delphi survey technique. journal of advanced nursing, 32(4), 1008–1015. henning, p.a. (2002). the examination of the newborn baby. pretoria: van schaik publishers. hodgman, j.e., hoppenbrouwers, t., & cabal, l.a. (1993). episodes of bradycardia during early infancy in the term-born and preterm infant. american journal of disabled children, 147(9), 980–984. http://dx.doi.org/10.1001/archpedi.1993.02160330050017 jadcherla, s. (2016). dysphagia in the high-risk infant: potential factors and mechanisms. american journal of clinical nutrition, 103(2), 622s–6288s. http://dx.doi.org/10.3945/ajcn.115.110106 karl, d.j. (2004). using principles of newborn behavioural state organization to facilitate breastfeeding. american journal of maternal child nursing, 29(5), 292–298. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00005721-200409000-00006 lau, c., & smith, e.o. (2011). a novel approach to assess oral feeding skills of preterm infants. neonatology, 100, 64–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000321987 meline, t. (2009). a research primer for communication sciences and disorders. boston, ma: allyn & bacon. national department of health. (2015). national consolidated guidelines for the prevention of mother-to-child transmission of hiv and the management of hiv in children, adolescents and adults. retrieved july 26, 2016, from: http://www.doh.co.za nugent, j.k. (2007). understanding newborn behaviour and early relationships: the newborn behavioural observations (nbo) systems handbook. baltimore, md: paul h. brookes publishing company. okoli, c., & pawlowski, s.d. (2004). the delphi method as a research tool: an example, design considerations and applications. information & management, 42, 15–29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.im.2003.11.002 pados, b.f., park, j., estrem, h., & awotwi, a. (2016). assessment tools for evaluation of oral feeding in infants younger than 6 months. advances in neonatal care, 16, 143–150. palmer, m.m., crawley, k., & blanco, i. (1993). neonatal oral-motor assessment scale: a reliability study. journal of perinatology, 13, 28–35. philbin, m.k., & ross, e. (2011). the soffi reference guides: text, algorithms, and appendices: a manualized method for quality bottle feedings. journal of perinatal and neonatal nursing, 25(4), 360–380. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/jpn.0b013e31823529da pike, c., pike, m., kritzinger, a., krüger, e., & viviers, m. (2016). risk profiles of infants with oro-pharyngeal and oesophageal dysphagia in a neonatal intensive care unit. south african journal of child health, 10(2), 130–133. http://dx.doi.org/10.7196/sajch.2016.v10i2.1051 prechtl, h.f.r., & beintema, d.j. (1964). the neurological examination of the full term newborn infant. clinics in developmental medicine, 12, 1–49. pressman, h. (2010). dysphagia and related assessment and management in children with hiv/aids. in d. swanepoel & b. louw (eds.). hiv/aids: related communication, hearing and swallowing disorders (pp. 351–384). oxfordshire: plural publishing. qureshi, m.a., vice, f.l., taciak, v.l., bosma, j.f., & gewolb, i.h. (2002). changes in rhythmic suckle feeding patterns in term infants in the first month of life. developmental medicine & child neurology, 44(1), 34–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0012162201001621 reilly, s., skuse, d., & wolke, d. (2000). soma: schedule for oral motor assessment. eastgardens, new south wales: whurr publishers, ltd. rogers, b., & arvedson, j. (2005). assessment of infant oral sensorimotor and swallowing function. mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews, 11, 74–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20055 rommel, n. (2006). assessment techniques for babies, infants and children. in j. cichero & b. murdoch, dysphagia: foundation, theory and practice (pp. 466–486). san diego, california: john wiley & sons, ltd. rudolph, c.d., & link, t.d. (2002). feeding disorders in infants and children. pediatric clinics of north america, 49(1), 97–112. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s0031-3955(03)00110-x schulz, v., kozell, k., biondo, p.d., stiles, c., martins, l., tonkin, k., et al. (2009). the malignant wound assessment tool: a validation study using a delphi approach. palliative medicine, 23(3), 266–273. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269216309102536 singh, s., booth, a., choto, f., gotlieb, j., robertson, r., morris, g., et al. (2015). new graduates perceptions of preparedness to provide speech-language therapy services in general and dysphagia in particular. south african journal of communication disorders, 62(1), 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v62i1.110 st pierre, a., khattra, p., johnson, m., cender, l., manzano, s., & holsti, l. (2010). content validation of the infant malnutrition and feeding checklist for congenital heart disease: a tool to identify risk of malnutrition and feeding difficulties in infants with congenital heart disease. journal of pediatric nursing, 25(5), 367–374. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.pedn.2009.04.009 strasheim, e., kritzinger, a., & louw, b. (2011). the development of a neonatal communication intervention tool. south african journal of communication disorders, 58, 13–18. swigert, n.b. (2010). the source for pediatric dysphagia. east moline, il: linguisystems. thoyre, s.m. (2007). feeding outcomes of extremely premature infants after neonatal care. journal of obstetetrics, gynecology and neonatal nursing, 36(4), 366–376. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1552-6909.2007.00158.x thoyre, s.m., park, j., pados, b., & hubbard, c. (2013). developing a co-regulated, cue-based feeding practice: the critical role of assessment and reflection. journal of neonatal nursing, 19, 139–148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jnn.2013.01.002 tsai, s.w., chen, c.h., & lin, m.c. (2010). prediction for developmental delay on neonatal oral motor assessment scale in preterm infants without brain lesion. pediatrics international, 52, 65–68. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1442-200x.2009.02882.x van haastert, i.c., de vries, l.s., helders, p.j.m., & jongmans, m.j. (2006). early gross motor development of preterm infants according to the alberta neonatal motor scale. journal of pediatrics, 149(5), 617–622. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jpeds.2006.07.025 wolff, p.h. (1959). observations on human infants. psychosomatic medicine, 221, 110–118. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00006842-195903000-00004 wolf, l.s., & glass, r.p. (1992). feeding and swallowing disorders in infancy: assessment and management. tucson, az: therapy skill builders. world health organization (who). (2001). international classification of functioning, disability and health (icidh-2). retrieved march 10, 2011, from http://www.who.int world health organization (who). (2012). born too soon. retrieved december 17, 2015, from http://www.who.int world health organization (who). (2010). guidelines on neonatal feeding. retrieved july 12, 2015, from http://www.who.int yousuf, m.i. (2007). using experts’ opinions through delphi-method. practical assessment, research & evaluation, 12(4), 1–7. the nature of jargon in the normal and language impaired child diane hurwitz, b . a . ( s p . & h . t h e r a p y ) ( w i t w a t e r s r a n d ) j.g. strijdom hospital, auckland park, johannesburg summary the jargon utterances of two groups of two subjects each, group a, children with normal speech and language development, and group b, children with delayed or impaired speech and language development, were recorded and transcribed. the data were divided into meaningful and non-meaningful categories. the former were analysed into morphemes in terms of distinctive features and phonemes. all subjects were, found to have essentially similar distinctive features, phonemes and morpheme structures with minor exceptions. intonation varied: group a used more sentence intonation, whereas group β used more word intonation. word approximations, standard and selflanguage words were found in all subjects. it was concluded that jargon appears to be a fusion of early phonological development and phonetic attempts, and that no significant difference exists between the two groups. opsomming die brabbeltaal van twee groepe bestaande uit twee proefpersone elk, groepp a, kinders met normale spraaken taalontwikkeling en groep b, met vertraagde spraak-en taalontwikkeling is opgeneem en neergeskryf. die gegewens is verdeel in sinvolle en nie-sinvolle kategorie. eersgenoemde is geanaliseer in morfeme in terme van onderskeidende kenmerke en foneme. daar is gevind dat al die proefpersone essensiele ooreenstemmende onderskeidende kenmerke het sowel as foneme en morfeme struktuur met klein verskille. intonasie het verskil. groep a het meer sinintonasie gebruik terwyl groep β meer woordintonasie gebruik het. woordapproximasies, standaard woorde, selftaalwoorde is gevind in alle proefkonyne. daar is tot die gevolgtrekking gekom dat brabbeltaal 'n vermenging van vroeer fonologiese ontwikkeling en fonetiese pogings skyn te wees en dat geen betekenisvolle verskil bestaan tussen die twee groepe nie. jargon is a type of infant utterance. these and other infant utterances are studied because it is assumed that there exists some correlation between prelanguage and language utterances and that these in some way prepare, determine or establish the bases for later language. also, their very nature e.g. a paucity or lack of vocalizations, may be used prognostically for later development. the investigator found the following characteristics or descriptions of jargon in the literature: tychkrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde. vol. 22. desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 diane hurwitz (1) "jargon", "gibberish", "unintelligible jabber" are all acceptable terms1·3·4· 6 , 1 2 , 1 3 , 1 5 , 1 8 (2) the earliest age of onset appears to be 3 months, with a peak at 18 months, and is usually gone by 2 y e a r s . 3 · 6 · 1 8 · 2 1 (3) it consists of sounds strung together, usually with repetition of morphemes but also varied sounds. 1 · 1 2 · 1 8 (4) characteristically it has adult intonation 1 · 1 2 · (5) it is unintelligble to the listener 5 · 1 2 (6) it appears to be non-symbolic. according to gesell & thompson 6 · the child is communicating but bloom & wyatt 2 1 · disagree. (7) it appears to be voluntary and controlled 1 , 1 2 (8) not all children use jargon 1 van r i p e r 1 8 · implies that the jargon of a child with delayed or impaired speech and language development would be similar to the above. clinically, some children using jargon have, on further analysis, been found to be using neologisms and/or having multiple articulatory errors and/or with impaired syntax. jargon must be distinguished from babbling. myklebust's17 definition that babbling is used: " . . . as the pleasureable use of vocalization . . . " appears apt. it begins at 3 months and extends to approximately 12 months which is the usual age when the first word is used. mowrer16 suggests that it is used primarily when the infant is contented, and not in the presence of the parent. experimental procedure hypothesis (1) the jargon of the child with normal speech and language development is an advanced stage of babbling. (2) the jargon of the child with delayed or impaired speech and language development will be different to (1) subjects there were two groups of two children each. group a were children with normal speech and language development. group β were children with delayed or impaired speech and language development. criteria for group a were that the child be between 1 5 and 19 months, that the intellectual and motor development appeared to be within normal limits (assessed subjectively from observation of child at play) and that the vocalizations were a gibberish with adult intonation. it did not matter if the child used words as well. the subjects were al, a girl of 17 months, who used only jargon; a2, a girl of 18 months, who used jargon and words. criteria for group β were that the child be older than 24 months, that a speech therapist diagnose the utterances as jargon, that intellectual and motor development may or may not be within normal limits. for this group jargon was not defined but obvious and gross articulatory and/or syntax errors were excluded. the subjects were, β1, a girl of 3 years, who used jargon and was at journal of the south african speech an hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) jargon in normal and language impaired children 65 the time of testing attending therapy; b2, a boy of 25 months who used jargon which was described by his parents as a "language of his own." all subjects had english as their home language. al's parents spoke hebrew as well, and bl's parents spoke hungarian. these foreign languages could have been an influence on the children and cannot be discounted. however, all the parents insisted that they only spoke english to their children, and that the foreign language was only for private interchange between the parents. socio-economic status was not considered but it did not appear to vary greatly. αι, a2 and b1 were all first children of the family. b2 was the third child. t e s t i n g a recording of the spontaneous utterances of each child was taken. the investigator visited each child at least once, so as to familiarise the child with herself and the taperecorder. the recordings were taken on one subsequent visit (except for a2, were recordings were taken over two visits). a sony portable/mains cassette taperecorder with an electret condenser microphone was used. the child was recorded in her bedroom whilst playing with toys and books provided by the investigator plus any other toys of her own. on most occasions the mother (or a sibling in the case of b2) was present at the recordings. the child played either with the mother or investigator. no attempt was made to correlate utterances with actions. if certain utterances appeared to have a target word or meaning, this was noted. the first 20-30 minutes of each recording for al, b1 and b2 were transcribed. for a2, her jargon utterances, plus about fifteen minutes of words were transcribed. the data were transcribed using a canon repeat-corder in broad phonetic notation using the symbols of the international phonetic alphabet, modified by the investigator with the help of professor l.w. lanham, department of phonetics and linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. the units of analysis were not phones i.e. speech sounds but phonemes, interpreted in terms of the investigator's english phonemic system. the symbols used were 'cover symbols' i.e. they stood for any sound that appeared to approximate that symbol eg /b/ [b, b, b h , p, p h ] . the symbol includes both voiced and voiceless cognates as one is unable to ascertain voicing without spectrographic analysis. the cover symbols used: stops: /b/ [b,b,bh,p,ph];/d/[d,d,dh,t,th];/g/[g,g,gh k, k h ] ; fricatives: /f/ [f, v] ;/s/ [s,z,x]; /s/ [s, z]; nasal resonants: /μ/ [ιη,φ]; /ν/ [η, η ο , α ] ; lateral resonants: /l/ [ 1, j ] ; any type of flag, trill or central resonant /r/; semivowels: /w/ ; /υ/ ; /h/ no affricates were used. consonant clusters were transcribed as consecutive consonants. τydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 diane hurwitz the cover symbols for the vowels: i 3 a e 9 u ε a , 3 p that is, four front vowels; high raised mid, lowered mid, and low; four central vowels: two mid, one low, and one central but rather back, three back vowels; high, mid and low. all front vowels were unrounded and all back vowels rounded. there were three glides a i , a,u, i 3 length was not considered. the boundaries of the utterances were"difficuluo define and was done subjectively. a pause, period of silence or an interruption by another speaker indicated a boundary. pause and stress were used to determine smaller units within the longer utterances. the transcriptions were transcribed twice, with an interval of two weeks, by the investigator. agreement was found. analysis of the results the data were divided into three categories using intonation as the distinguishing factor: (1) clear, distinct utterances. (2) vague utterances eg. musings, wailings, singing. (3) exclamations and interjections. categories 2 and 3 were discarded and category 1 was sub-divided: certian shorter sequences appeared to be maximally stressed within a longer utterance and bounded on either side by silence, however minimal the period. these were called morphemes after the definition by gleason7. this term was chosen in preference to word which implies a sound-symbol relationship. a morpheme was composed of any number of syllables which was usually a consonant followed by a vowel; very rarely a vowel occurred in isolation as a syllable, but then it was always followed by a consonant and a vowel. each consonant and vowel was a phoneme as discribed by jakobson 1 1 : "a bundle" of distinctive features i.e. those features that establish contrast between phonemes. certain of the morphemes could be recognized as "babytalk" or standard english words and were called such, and included in the data for analysis. this approach was adopted because lanham1 4 feels that morphemes are phonetic attempts by the child to achieve a target word which would then be stressed. bloom1 postulates that the child during the jargon period is practising adult intonation. thus, as an adult would stress an important word, so the child stresses the meaningful morphemes. it is felt, and gruber9 supports this, that the child appears to be speaking language as opposed to babbling and he found that the morphemes corresponded to those of english. subjectively, the fact that all children already used recognizable words and that they demanded attention and responses from their listeners gives support for this approach. if, as it is postulated, the child is attempting to convey meaning then he will be setting up oppositions in the form of a system i.e. distinctive features. j a k o b s o n ' s 1 0 · 1 1 distinctive features were modified by the investigator on the suggestion of lanham 1 4 . only articulatory features i.e. of place and manner journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) jargon in normal and language impaired children 67 were considered. they are as follows: each is binary, either a phoneme has that feature (+) or it does not (—). (1) vocalic / non-vocalic: where there is no obstruction of the airflow by the organs of articulation. (2) consonantal / non-consonantal: either complete or incomplete obstruction. (3) continuous / non-continuous: partial obstruction. (4) resonant / non-resonant: no friction and partial obstruction. (5) nasalized / non-nasalized: air flows through the nose, obstruction in the mouth. (6) lateral / non-lateral: air flows over the sides of the tongue. (7) front / non-front: sound is formed at the front of the mouth. (8) back / non-back: sound is formed at the back of the mouth. (9) low / non-low: the tongue is low in the mouth. (10) glide / non-glide: first vowel is followed by a movement toward another vowel. voicing was not recognized as a phonetic feature and thus is not included as a distinctive feature. the distinctive features are organized into phonemes as follows: /b/ /d/ ,/g/ + consonantal — vocalic — continuous + front + consonantal — vocalic — continuous — front _ + consonantal^ — vocalic — continuous + back if i /s/ in eld. 3 + consonantal — vocalic + continuous — resonant + front _ + consonantal — vocalic + continuous — resonant — front — back _ /m/ /n/ + consonantal — vocalic + continuous + resonant + nasalized + front + consonantal — vocalic + continuous + resonant + nasalized — front — back __ ydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 8 diane hurwitz / l/ + consonantal — vocalic + continuous + resonant — nasalized + lateral — front — back /r/ + consonantal — vocalic + continuous + resonant — nasalized — lateral — front — back /w/ + consonantal + vocalic + continuous — resonant — front λ7 + consonantal + vocalic + continuous — resonant — front _ / h / + consonantaf 4vocalic + continuous — resonant + back _ / i / [ i , e , e , 3 , 3 ] — consonantal + vocalic + front — low i [u,d, a] — consonantal + vocalic + back — low _ / a / [ a e a , p ] — consonantal + vocalic — front — back + low _ the data were re-written in terms of the phonemic system. results and discussion morpheme structure the four most common morpheme structures were: cv, cvc, cvcv, vcv where the vowel (v) and the consonant (c) may o'r may not be identical with the morpheme. ' in group a cvcv appeared to be the most common form, closely followed by cvc; whereas for group b, cv was clearly the most common form; cvc and journal of the south african speech anil hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) jargon in normal and language impaired children 69 cvcv occurred less often, but more or less equally. vcv was used infrequently by a2, b1 and b2 and not at all by a1. all subjects excluding b2 had an additional idiosyncratic structure: a1 was cv l : where either /l/ can be regarded an established phoneme, thus cvc, or as a phonetic realisation of the vowel, thus cv. neither can be stated emphatically. a similar case occurred with b1 and cvn. . a.2 used ccv: either cj c2 v or cc can be recognized as a single element and c2 is listed as a distinctive feature of c r in this manner a new morpheme category does not have to be created. this notation can be used to simplify other morphemes used by β1 eg vcc vc; cvcc cvc, and others. only certain consonants appeared as c 2 so that it appears that this explanation is better than postulating the existence of clusters. if b1 did in fact have consonant clusters then it would be evidence of his abnormal development. burlings2, velten1 9, and winitz & irwin2 0 found similar morpheme structures for the period 16-21 months. distinctive features, phonemes and phonemic systems the following distinctive features were strongly established in all subjects: [± vocalic], [±consonantal], [±continuous], [±resonant], [±front], [±back], [±low], [+ nasal], [ glide]. the following are weakly or in the process of being established: [ nasal], [+ lateral], [+ glide]. only b1 used [ lateral] and it appears to be weakly established. the phonemic system common to all subjects was the following: consonants: /b/ /d/ /g/ ' /si ' /n/ ili /w/ vowels /i/ /u/ /a/ the strongest opposition was that of the consonants and vowels. this is the first opposition that the child learns.10· n all subjects showed strong opposition between non-continuants (i.e. stops) and continuants (i.e. fricatives and resonants). stops were the predominant type of consonant, and the only type to contrast in three places of articulation. for 3 subjects (excluding b2) nasal resonants seemed better established than fricatives because they contrasted both labially and dentally. b.2 used only a dental nasal: this violates jakobson's law of solidarity which states that one cannot have a [ front] before a [+ front] consonant. the reasons for this may be that the data were insufficient, inaccuracy of transcription or impairment of development. because fricatives occurred so rarely, and particularly labial fricatives it seems that the principle of "underarticulation" may be applied i.e. the child is aiming at a stop but produces a fricative. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vercniging vir spraak cn gelworhcclkumle, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 diane hurwitz the results appear to be in agreement with j a k o b s o n 1 0 , 1 1 who states that the first opposition among type is stop vs. nasal, and not velten1 9 who states that it is stop vs. fricative. the acquisition of non-nasal resonants is a late acquisition: all subjects used /l/ infrequently. b1 used both /r/ and /l/ and this is usually an extremely late acquisition and is probably further evidence of his impaired development. all subjects used /w/ and this agrees with previous findings10·19 which indicate that it is the first semi-vowel to emerge. only a1 did not use /y/ and this is probably because she was younger and at an earlier stage of development. /h/ is difficult to transcribe and may have been confused with a voiceless vowel. b1 used it once, b2 more frequently. its absence in group a may be queried as velten1 9 found it established at 16 months. the many varied phonetic realizations of the vowels were fitted into jakobson's 1 0 , 1 1 primary triangle: front back i u a low the status of the vowels is difficult to ascertain, but if one assumes that vowel and consonant development run concurrently then it would appear that the oppositions do exist. however, goldstein8 states that at a certain period only the c is contrastive, this may be so in view of the word approximations. [+ glide] is weakly established: two glides were used by all subjects. burlings2 reported that his child used them at 18-19 months. the results with minor exceptions appear to be confirmed by j a k o b s o n ' s 1 0 , 1 1 studies. the distinctive features are similar to those of joan velten1 9 at 16 months with the exception that the investigator has set up three place distinctions instead of two. word approximations in.these attempts the child would variously try different vowels and consonants, but each was clearly recognizable eg. didi did8 bidi /gigi/ [kiti] kitty gidi gagi this seems to be strong support for the postulation that the other morphemes are also some sort of variation of some unknown target word. words these included both standard and self language words: all subjects had them but the number varied: a1 two, a2 many, β1 four, b2 only self language word eg. nana for mother. journal of the south african speech an hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) jargon in normal and language impaired children 71 intonation group a used recognizable english sentence intonation, with the occasional question intonation. a2 used word intonation when using words. group β used the same type of intonation as group a but to a lesser extent; they tended to give each morpheme separate intonation. intonation observed in group a confirms similar observations made by bloom.1 conclusion hypothesis 1 is rejected because it seems that the jargon of the child with normal speech and language development appears to be a fusion of the early stages of phonological development and phonetic attempts at a target, possibly occuring either before or simultaneously to the acquisition of the first words i.e. it is postulated that jargon belongs not to the pre-language but to the language period or alternatively represents a bridge between the two. it seems doubtful though that jargon can be said to be a language. hypothesis 2 cannot be accepted nor rejected: the jargon of the two groups appears to be essentially the same; if this is so, then group β may be said simply to have delayed speech. but if the various deviations discussed are accepted as such then one can conclude that group b's development is impaired. the implications of this study appear to be that if a child is using jargon beyond two years, his speech and language development is delayed but that he does seem to be able to learn oppositions. thus diagnosis should indicate those contrasts that he has and therapy directed towards those he has not. it would seem most appropriate to begin therapy at the holophrastic or twoword stage. references 1. bloom, 1. (1970): language development: form and function in emerging grammars. the mit press, cambridge, massachusetts and london. 2. burling, r. (1959): language development of a garo and english speaking child. word, 15, 45-68. , 3. cameron, j., livson, n., bayley, n. (1967): infant vocalizations and their relationship to mature intelligence. science, 157, 331-333. 4. concise oxford dictionary, 5th ed. 5. day, e.j. (1932): the development of language in twins: comparison of twins and single children. child development. 3, 3, 179-199. 6. gesell, a. & thompson, h. (1934): infant behaviour: its genesis and growth. mcgraw-hill book co., inc. new york & london. 7. gleason, h.a. (1961): an introduction to descriptive linguistics. (revised edition) holt, rinehart & winston, inc. new york. 8. goldstein, k. (1948): language and language disturbances. grune & stratton, new york. 9. gruber, j.s. (1973): playing with distinctive features in the babbling ydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 22, desember 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 diane hurwitz of infants. in studies of child language development. ferguson, c.a. & slobin, d.i. (eds.) holt, rinehart & winston, inc. new york. 10. jakobson, r. (1968): child language, aphasia and phonological universals. mouton & co., n.v. publishers, the hague. 11. jakobson, r. & halle, m. (1956): fundamentals of language. mouton & co., 'sgravenhage. 12. jones, m.v. (1972): language development: the key to learning. • charles c. thomas. 13. kornfeld, j.r. (1971): theoretical issues in child phonology. paper from the seventh regional meeting of the chicago linguistic society, april 16th-18th. chicago linguistic society, chicago, illinois. 14. lanham, l.w. (1974): personal communication. 15. milisen, r. (1966): articulatory problems. chap. 13 m speech pathology. rieber, r.w. & brubaker, r.s. (eds.). north holland publishing co., amsterdam. 16. mowrer, o.h. (1958): hearing and speaking: an analysis of language learning./. speech. heardis., 23, 143-152. 17. myklebust, h.r. (1957): babbling and echolalia in language theory. j. speech hear. dis., 22, 3, 356-60. 18. van riper, c. (1972): speech correction: principles and methods. (5th ed.) constable & co., ltd., london. 19. velten, h.v. (1943): the growth of phonemic and lexical patterns in infant language. language, 19, 281-292. 20. winitz, h. & irwin o.c. (1958): syllabic and phonetic structure of infants' early words./. speech. hear. res., 1, 250-256. 21. wyatt g.l. (1969): language learning & communication disorders in children. collier macmillan, canada ltd., toronto, ontario. journal of the south african speech an hearing association, vol. 22, december 1975 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) brclel & kjaer manufacturers of electro-acoustical instrumentation * audiometer calibration * hearing aid test sets * sound level meters * noise dose meters * artificial ears * artificial voice * artificial mastoid * frequency analysers for more information write or phone: sole s.a. agents: — telkor electronics (pty.) ltd. p.o. box 7764 johannesburg 200 telephone jhbg.: 836-1301 a l s o : dowson & dobson ltd. + durban — cape town — port elizabeth + member of the afrox group. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ii surgical procedures for the deafness due to otosclerosis d. r. h a y n e s , m.b., b.ch., (rand), d.l.o. (r.c.p. & s.) ear, nose and throat surgeon the history of otosclerosis is of interest and importance for the appreciation of modern surgical techniques. more than in any other branch of surgery is the technique and its meticulous execution able to determine the difference between success and failure in restoring hearing to the sufferer from the stapesfixing process of otosclerosis. modern operations on the temporal bone depend fundamentally on a knowledge of the anatomy of this intricate part of the body and i think it is safe to say that more has been learnt of the physiology of hearing since lempert established the fenestration operation than we knew before. the anatomy of the ear is most easily understood if represented diagrammatically and the accompanying sketches attempt this. for the convenience of describing the physiology of hearing, the anatomy is divided into three parts — the outer, the middle and the inner ears. the inner ear consists of the acoustic nerve endings and it is sufficient here to say that from the point of view of the surgery of otosclerosis the acoustic nerve must be intact before surgery can hope to succeed. f i g u r e 1 the middle ear consists of three small bones or ossicles which form a chain connecting the drum membrane with the oval window. by this means, sound vibrations which enter the outer ear are transmitted from the tympanic membrane via the ossicular chain to the oval window. the smallest of the three ossicles, the stapes or stirrup, consists of two legs or crura attached to an oval plate which is capable of moving like a trapdoor in the oval window. the trapdoor is hinged at its posterior end and the maximum excursions take place at the anterior end. it is important to appreciate this fact as will be seen when the pathology of otosclerosis is discussed. since the inner ear is filled with fluid and this is incompressible, vibrations of the stapes footplate require an outlet and this is provided by another window covered by a membrane and known as the round window. normally, sound waves enter the external ear canal and produce vibrations of the tympanic memberane. these are in turn transmitted through the ossicular chain to the oval window where vibrations are set (yj^dph fig. 1 fig. 2 otosclerosis journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) in motion in the fluid of the inner ear. it is these vibrations that stimulate the nerve endings in the inner ear and impulses are conveyed to the brain. f i g u r e 2 in the condition known as otosclerosis, there is an overgrowth of spongy bone starting at the anterior end ol the oval window and progressively involving the neighbouring structures. when the spongy bone encroaches on the footplate of the stapes, this structure becomes fixed and, as a result, is prevented from transmitting sound vibrations brought to it by the ossicular chain. as the condition progresses, the whole of the footplate may be involved and even the posterior end may become fixed although, as mentioned above, maximum movement takes place at the anterior end and it is here that the condition produces its maximum interference with hearing. versalius first described the two larger bones in the middle ear cavity in the sixteenth century and the stapes was described by ingrassis at a later date. it says a great deal for the observational powers of valsalva that he described fixation of the stapes as long ago as 1735 and we may be sure that his observation was not assisted in any way by any of the instruments that we consider essential for this type of miniature anatomy at the present time. it was more than a century later that toynbee recognised stapes ankylosis to be the cause of deafness and although his observations were not accepted without misgivings and reluctance by many of his colleagues, we have records of attempts to mobilize the stapes footplates for the relief of deafness in 1878. the first operations failed because of re-fixation of the ossicle and in 1897 passouw tried to by-pass the oval window by making a fenestra in the promontory but surgical procedures in this region were considered to be unjustified for the relief of deafness because of the risk of infection of the inner ear and secondary infection of the cranial cavity. in 1913 jenkins by-passed the oval window by creating a fenestra in the lateral semicircular canal and obtained1 an immediate improvement in hearing which unfortunately was short-lived. once again the surgeons were up against the serious risk of uncontrollable infection. modifications in technique and the advent of antibiotics led eventually to the revolutionary one-stage fenestration nov-ovalis of dr. julius lempert whose tenacity of purpose triumphed over the tremendous opposition of his fellow-otologists all over the world and established surgery of the aural labyrinth on a firm footing. not only did his work indicate surgical possibilities with the use of modern lighting and magnification but it also led to the refutation of many of the previously held theories of the physiology of hearing and enabled great advances to be made in the field of audiology. the ramifications of these advances are almost limitless and when one begins to speculate on the newly charted seas of otology since lempelt's first successful fenestration operation in 1938, the magnitude of his contribution to the world at large can be to a small extent appreciated. f i g u r e 3 the lempert fenestration operation by-passed the fixed foot-plate of the stapes and a new fenestra was made in the lateral semi-circular canal. the new fenestra was then covered by a flap of tissue consisting of the skin of the external canal attached to the tympanic membrane and in order to obtain the most perfect application of this tissue to the new fenestra the incus and the head of the malleus were removed. since the oval window was being by-passed, the ossicular chain was of course not required for its normal physiological function but because the mechanical advantage of the chain was lost the ultimate hearing level was also depressed to a small extent below normal. fig. 3 f e n e s t r a t i o n june, 1961 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the fenestration operation reached a very high peak of technical perfection and the indications for operation were worked out to such a fine degree that predictions on the ultimate result could be given with a great degree of accuracy. in 1952, samuel rosen, employing the fundamental techniques of lempert, revived the operation for stapes mobilization and obtained considerable success. modifications were not long in appearing and because the operation was rapidly completed with very little discomfort to the patient it naturally had a tremendous appeal to the victims of otosclerosis. experience has shown that of all the stapes mobilization operations undertaken only 30 to 40 per cent can be anticipated to give good permanent results. as experience has increased in this operation, more and more hazards have come to light but in many cases it has been found that if the procedure is not successful or re-fixation of the stapes footplate occurs, further surgery may be undertaken. in those cases that failed after an initial improvement in hearing, rosen sometimes perforates the promontory and this procedure gives a very temporary improvement in some cases. f i g u r e 4 the operation for stapes mobilization is performed through the external ear canal and part of the skin of the canal attached to the tympanic membrane is elevated and reflected forwards. this exposes the middle ear cavity with its ossicles and the foot-plate of the stapes may be examined directly. mobilization can be effected by inserting a needle into the head of the stapes and rocking it back and forth until the adhesions of otosclerotic bone around the footplate are broken down. if this procedure does not succeed, mobilization of the footplate is achieved by exerting pressure on the footplate directly. it may be necessary in some cases to use a small chisel to break down a large focus of spongy bone around the anterior part of the window. when mobilization has been successfully achieved, it is usually possible to see the round window reflex. this consists of movement of the round window membrane on applying pressure to the stapes footplate. when 60 per cent of mobilized stapes began to re-fix, it became obvious that the mobilization operation would have to be altered while the principle of maintaining the anatomy of the middle ear should be adhered to. for those cases in which the otosclerotic process was confined to the anterior edge of the stapes footplate, fowler amputated the anterior cms of the stapes and fractured the footplate across its centre. the ossicular chain was in this manner maintained 16 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) and vibrations could be transmitted through the posterior eras of the stapes. the operation is known as an anterior crurotomy. it gave a higher percentage of permanently successful results than the operation for stapes mobilization. f i g u r e 5 john shea, jnr., of memphis, tennessee, discussed the general principles of stapes mobilization with his orthopaedic colleagues and concluded that the procedures suggested up to that time were unphysiological and doomed for the most part to failure. he then decided to remove the pathological stapes and its footplate and replace them with prostheses. the oval window was considered to be the natural pathway for the transmission of sound waves to the inner ear and apart from anything else the chances of closure of a natural window in the labyrinth seemed to be smaller than the chances in the case of an artificial fenestra. cawthorne at king's college hospital had already attempted a modification of the lempert fenestration using the oval window but had encountered the difficulty of obtaining good application of the tympanic membrane over the window so it was obvious that a substitute for the stapes must be found. f i g u r e 6 shea's stapedectomy consists of removal of the stapes with its footplate and covering the oval window with a vein graft. connection between this and the incus is then effected by inserting a polythene tube between the articulating process of the long process of the incus and the vein graft lying over the oval window. so far, the results have shown 90 per cent success to the bone conduction level of hearing and most of the patients operated on by shea four years ago (when the operation was first performed) have maintained their improvement. needless to say, the operation had hardly been shown to be successful before "improvements" were suggested embodying the sound principles of the original operation. house uses a plug of gelatin sponge suspended from the incus by a piece of stainless steel wire and projecting into the oval window. schucknecht uses a small piece of fat or muscle cut from the ear in preference to the gelatin sponge and suspends it in a similar way in the oval window, i f i g u r e 7 portman of; bordeaux employs a vein graft to cover the oval window and uses the patient's own posterior stapedial crus to complete the bridge between the incus and the oval window. ruedi of zurich removes the stapes completely and then replaces the stapes fig. 6 s h e a s t a p e d e c t o m y fig. 7 s c h u c k n e c h t s t a p e d e c t o r y ι june, 1961 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ."crura together with the articulating process to act as his prosthesis. cawthorne uses a twisted steel wire for the prosthesis bridging the stapedial gap. f i g u r e 8 some surgeons have been pessimistic about the future of this type of surgery, but it would seem that, at the risk of the unfortunate exception suffering irreversible damage to the hearing in one ear, one must have the courage to attempt the restoration of normal hearing to the patients suffering from otosclerosis. book i w i e w g h e a r i n g a n d deafness h a l l o w e l d a v i s and s. r i c h a r d s i l v e r m a n , editors holt, rinehart and winston, inc., new york, 1960 as a new science developing from its parent studies becomes an independent speciality, a new vocabulary and literature arises. so the new word "audiology" describing the science of hearing with all its experimental and clinical implications, came into being in the mid-forties, and the need for a text like hearing and deafness edited by hallowell davis (1947) arose. this book served to discuss most of the aspects of hearing and the problems of deafness, and the extensive knowledge and authority of the many contributors fulfilled the criterion established by the author: this book is written for the deaf and the hard of hearing and for their families, their parents, their teachers, and their friends. it is written for physicians, for educators, for social workers, and for all who are concerned with the conservation or improvement of remaining hearing or with the approach to normal living for those who have suffered either complete or partial hearing loss. it is written to answer the thousand and one questions that are continually being asked by all sorts of people about the nature of hearing and the problems posed by partial or complete loss of hearing. audiology has developed, both in depth and extent, so rapidly, and the editor of hearing and deafness is so aware of the needs created by this growth that, thirteen years after the first edition, a revised version has been published with dr. s. richard silverman as co-editor to dr. davis. the shift of emphasis in this edition is implicit in the omission of the sub-title "a guide for laymen". although the layman with impaired hearing, or with children who are deaf or hard of hearing, will still fig. 8 c a w t h o r n e s t a p e d e c t o m y fig. 9 t h e e u s t a c h i a n t u b e a drawing from a dissection of the eustachian tube showing its relationship to the middle ear space on one end and the nasopharynx on the other end. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ϋηά the answers to his questions, the main orientation is to students of audiology, and as such, makes this text essential to those readers who, like speech therapists, come to audiology from a closely allied but differently orientated field. as in the first edition, the sequence of the book is from inanimate nature to the individual human to complex social problems — from physical to biological to social. the editors describe this sequence as being "from physics, biology, medicine and surgery to modern studies of impaired hearing and hearing aids, and thence to special education and rehabilitation of adults with impaired hearing. the problems of the education of deaf and hard of hearing children are discussed next, and then the organised social efforts on behalf of the aurally handicapped; and finally, employment and vocational guidance." the second edition presents some new authors and aspects; and re-organization and addition of knowledge and viewpoints within the other chapters. some of these may be mentioned briefly as they serve to show how the altered scope of this edition has served to cover the enlarged audiological field. in his chapter on "the physics and psychology of hearing", dr. davis introduces the cencept of a threshhold zone, and, while discussing anatomy and physiology of the ear, elaborates on the biophysics and physiology of the inner ear, which are now better understood. the description and causes of the various impairments of hearing have been re-organised into a new chapter, called "hearing and deafness", and it is here that dr. davis and dr. fowler propose a more useful set of definitions for hearing impairments than those which existed before. the chapter on the medical aspects of hearing is brought up to date by dr. fowler, and the rapid development of treatment and prevention provides much new material, as does the new section on hearing conservation. surgical treatment has advanced much in the last decade, and details of the stapes mobilization, as well as the fenestration, are given by dr. walsh. "tests of hearing" and the chapter, new in this edition, on special auditory tests, will be of the greatest practical value to speech therapists interested in audiology, as will the discussion on the rehabilitative aspects of | the problem, given in the detailed and •explicit chapters on hearing aids, and their choice and use, together iwith dr. miriam pauls' section on speech reading and dr. carhart's on auditory training and the conservation of speech. the rehabilitative procedures with deaf and hard of hearing children are dealt with in more detail by the authors in this edition and the psychological, sociological and vocational aspects of the hearing-impaired are discussed in the light of increased knowledge of these problems. if the original edition was useful and informative, this edition can be considered invaluable. the thousand and one questions posed not only by the layman, but by the workers and allied professional workers in the new field, are admirably answered. hearing and deafness is a text which is indispensable to the student of audiology. margaret marks, m.a. stroke. a d i a r y of recovery by d o u g l a s r i t c h i e faber and faber, london, 1960 174 pages douglas ritchie was a well-known announcer on the b.b.c. when he suffered a severe cerebral haemorrhage in 1955. he was left with a right hemiplegia and severe aphasia. this book, started two years later, is his account of what happened to him, as he remembers it. although his language is of a relatively high standard, it is evident that his recovery is not complete. the narrative is at times confused, and it is difficult to see the point of certain passages. the book is nevertheless a remarkable achievement. it is a record of the thoughts that were going through this man's mind when he was virtually speechless, and should add much to our understanding of the aphasic's feelings. mr. ritchie's comments on speech therapy are revealing : "i liked miss f. very much, but loathed the time i used to spend at speech therapy." "every student seemed to like the reader's digest, and i had to read the first few paragraphs of many articles". "miss b's influence was not confined to speech, or language re-education as one might better call it. victims of ahasia did want to regain the power of language, but, above that, nearly all of them unconsciously craved for some emotional balance of which they had been robbed by the stroke. ability to help in this need was miss b's real quality." many aphasic patients, too, would benefit from reading the story of mr. richie's battle against great odds and his gradual adjustment to his difficulties. he also gives practical suggestions to other aphasics, and discusses some of the theoretical aspects of aphasia. he feels that by gaining an understanding of his condition, he had a better idea of the purpose of the various therapists who were working with him. pat allsopp. june, 196 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society june case history of a brain-injured child (taken from notes on a demonstration given at the vacation course on "therapies in cerebral palsy" in johannesburg, october, 1957. reprinted with the permission of the national council for the care of cripples in south africa. by c. ordman, b.a. log (rand|) johannesburg school and treatment centre for cerebral palsied children. m. is a child who is not motor handicapped but who may be diagnosed as "brain'injured" on the basis of his history and symptoms. his behaviour may also be described as "autistic" (!) ( 2 ) . he is now four years eight months of age. history: his mother is r.h. negative. she had a. "blackout" during pregnancy and whooping cough in the ninth month. m. was a full term baby and weighed 6 lb. 11 oz. he held up his head at three months and sat up at seven months. at this stage he began to make queer movements of his arms and trunk, and these were diagnosed as "fits." initially, they occurred after mealtimes, but gradually their frequency increased. vomiting started at about the same time, and he began losing weight. at approximately twelve months he was hospitalised for two weeks. his condition continued to deteriorate. an e.e.g. taken at this time showed an abnormal record. an airstudy "seemed to indicate slight atrophy of the frontal part of the brain." (verbal report). he was described as hyperkinetic and overactive, and frequent head-dropping was noted. fits and vomiting continued until he was 16 months of age. his general condition then improved and he attempted to stand. at 20 months, the e.e.g. was repeated and again an abnormal record was found "but with considerable improvement." at this stage he was walking. he did not develop speech, took no notice of people, and was unmanageable. a third e.e.g. taken 17 months later showed "a normal record of cortical activity for his age." he was admitted to a school for cerebral palsied children in may, 1955, at the age of two years five months. observations behaviour. he stood with his legs apart rocking from side to side and humming. he was extremely disorganised. he did not use objects correctly. he would for example, upset his milk and then rub it in his hair. he was hyperactive in a purposeless destructive way. unless all cupboard doors were locked, he would inevitably fling the contents in all directions. he stamped on toys and pushed chairs over. he appeared to be unable to control this behaviour. when his activities were limited, he became very distressed. he beat his head, threw himself on the floor and uttered short cries. in the nursery school he had to be watched continually and a special nanny came to school with him every day. social response. he took very little notice of people and his facial expression seldom changed. he smiled only when he was pleasing himself or when he was being tickled. he never looked directly at anyone. when his attention was arrested, he stood looking with his eves down, and his head to the side. speech. he was usually silent, but babbled a little while throwing toys. again, he seemed to babble for his own satisfaction rather than for communication. the aims of therapy, were therefore:— 1) to establish a relationship with him. 2) to decrease his disorganised activity and make him less disturbed. 3) to stimulate his speech production and provide him with an atmosphere in which he would want to communicate. 1 shall discuss these three aspects of therapy separately, although i believe they are closely related. treatment 1) establishing a relationship with him. the therapist tried to make him feel that somebody was warm and affectionate towards him. he was cuddled most of the time, and spoken to in a calm, reassuring voice. in a concrete way a co-operative relationship was built up by rolling a ball to him and gently making him roll it back. similarly, the therapist would do a jigsaw puzzle and expect or aid him to pass her the pieces. he was encouraged to respond to relevant speech such as "give it to me", "pick it up" and "do you want a sweet?" r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) june journal of the south african logopedic society 2. decreasing his disorganised activity and making him less disturbed. limits were set regarding destruction and times for therapy. these were applied unemotionally and it was made clear that he would be punished. for destructive behaviour, but that the therapist still approved of him and loved him. he learnt that one behaved differently with different materials and that whereas he could not tear books, he could scribble on paper and knock down blocks. at first each session followed a pattern. m. knew that as soon as he came into the room he would have to sit down and perform an organised activity. after that he was allowed to play as he liked, until the therapist looked at her watch and said: "time to go now." using the watch may have acted as an objective factor of organised behaviour. m. knew that he could rely on the therapist not to interfere with his play as long as he was not destructive. this trust became mutual as he gained control. he was no longer watched so carefully and the therapist showed tthat she expected certain responses. this approach was carried over to the nursery school where his teacher expected him to take his turn serving milk to the other children. the occupational therapist helped him to play constructively by providing him with material which required purposeful activity, and which incidentally taught matching and discrimination of forms, sizes and colours. 3) stimulating speech. the therapist spoke to him at every opportunity without demanding, but expecting, speech from him. all his large movements and vocalisations were imitated, so that he would begin to imitate other people. he heard words such as "open" "close" "pull" "push" "up" "dwn" in their relevant contexts, and he began to repeat them. the therapist vocalised rhythmically as she drew regular patterns, and again m. imitated. his mother was given an outline of therapy and was asked to continue these activities at home. results m. has progressed well in every way. he now responds to people, communicates by smiling and speech, and reacts to instructions. socially he is becoming a real member of the nursery school group, and is beginning to take part in ring activities., he is fortunate in having understanding and . well-trained nursery school teachers who are able to assess his readiness for new responsibilities. if he is occasionally destructive it is usually because he is angry, and he is able to control this reaction. his play is organised and he concentrates for longer periods. he uses toys and different materials meaningfully. he is sufficiently organised, e.g. to turn the tap on, fill the basin with water, turn the tap off, select the relevant toys (boats, duck and fish) and sail them. at first he tended to repeat the same activity over and over again. as he progressed he not only chose different play materials, but used the same materials in different ways. on the emotional side, his facial expression now reflects joy, anger, recognition, mischief and so on. he uses speech to express his needs and to communicate. he shows interest in naming objects and his vocabulary is growing. he is beginning to look through picture books. he says three word sentences like "look a pic" (look at the picture) and "i wa a tic" (i want a stick). he enjoys meaningful sounds and repeats immediately that the car says "r-r-r" and the scissors "cut". as far as psychological assessment is concerned, this has always been difficult because of his behaviour disorder. the psychologist's report on a test given four months ago, was as follows: "m. has improved but it is still not possible to make an accurate assessment of his mental ability as he is too distractible and only able to concentrate for ten minutes at a time." he did, however, pass the sequin formboard and the wallin peg-board tests at the 36-41 months level, and he was able to correct his errors. summary in summary, i have described a child with history of probable brain damage. he had symptoms of disorganisation, temper tantrums, disturbance in social contact, . destructibility, hyperactivity, lack of concentration and possible perseveration. some of these symptoms may be associated with a diagnosis of autism. he is at a combined school and treatment centre and is making excellent progress. it will be some time before he is ready for formal school work, and it remaiv.s to be seen whether he will have special learning difficulties. befebences: (1). kanner "early infantile autism" journal of paediatrics, vol. 25 no. 3,. sept. 1944. (2) kanner, lee: "the conception of wholes and parts in early infantile autism." am. f journal of psychiatry, july 1951. vol. 108. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational grammar and the case of an ndebele speaking aphasic a . traill department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand introduction there is a refrain running through much of current linguistic theorizing which says that language is rule-governed behaviour. if i am not mistaken there is an uneasiness running through much of psychology and speech therapy which is based on the feeling that this linguistic view of language is somehow of great relevance to the understanding of normal and abnormal verbal behaviour but just what this relevance is, is not at all clear. this state of affairs is understandable if one bears in mind the following facts: firstly, linguistics is not a unified discipline. its practitioners agree that what they are doing is studying the structure of .human language, but beyond this there is little agreement as to what this structure looks like, whether it is universal and, very importantly, how one should formalize it. secondly, linguists study normal language and furthermore their descriptions are descriptions of langue in saussure's sense or competence in chomsky's sense. that is to say linguists qua linguists have nothing to say about how language is used (parole, performance) in listening, reading or speaking and writing, let alone describing the effects pathologies can have on these normal performances. all this is not to say that linguistics has had no influence in psychology or speech therapy. one finds phonemes, morphemes and syntax cropping up in both these fields and in a number of instances, more than lip service is paid to these concepts. it is no doubt due to the influence of men like r. jakobson that we find linguistic concepts circulating amongst aphasiologists (cf. jakobson 1964) and those concerned with language acquisition (cf. jakobson 1968), although one suspects that his influence should have been greater than it has been. quite recently, the so-called chomskyan revolution in linguistics has had repercussions in related fields, giving new direction to the interdisciplinary field of psycholinguistics (for a summary of research see fodor, bever and garrett 1968) and has given birth to what d. mcneill terms developmental psycholinguistics (examples of this new approach to the acquisition of language can be found in mcneill, 1966, bellugi and brown, 1964, menyuk, 1969). chomsky has stated on more than one occasion that linguistic theory is a psychological theory to the extent that it attempts to provide a formalization of the intuitive linguistic knowledge (competence) of the native speaker of a human language and journal of the south african logopedic' society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : t h e case of an ndebele speaking aphasic 49 it is therefore not surpising to find linguistics having an impact on other disciplines. there is to my knowledge only one published attempt to apply the new theory literally to the field of language disorders and this is marshall and newcombe's study of syntactic and semantic errors in paralexia (marshall and newcombe, 1966). had the new theory stood still for long enough we would doubtless have had more examples of its application to the analysis of language disturbances in adults and children. the rationale underlying this recent work in psycholinguistics is as follows: the grammar of a language is a formalization of the intuitive linguistic knowledge which a native speaker of a language internalizes in the course of learning his language and which constrains what is to count as acceptable performance in the language. the formalization is effected by various types of rules operating at various levels of representation: the phonological level, the syntactic level and the semantic level. the theory provides a mechanism for mapping one level of representation onto another, but not in any order: the syntactic level is regarded as central in standard transformational theory and the syntactic representations are mapped onto semantic and phonological representations. the theory thus provides an integrated formalization of the informal observation that sentences in a language have pronunciation (phonological representation), meaning (semantic representation) and syntax (syntactic representation). furthermore, the pairings of meanings and pronunciations (sounds) with syntax mediating, is over an infinite domain. that is to say, one of the facts about native speaker competence which is accounted for in the theory, is that a native speaker of a language is, in principle, able to comprehend and produce an infinite number of sentences including typically ones which are wholly novel. put slightly differently, the grammar of a language provides a definition of the infinite set of grammatical sentences in that language. what the psycholinguists have done, is to regard this theory as a source for formulating hypotheses about linguistic performance. they have, in other words, attempted to predict psychological complexity from complexity in a grammar. if a certain sentence has η rules in its derivation in the grammar, and another sentence as n + 2 rules in its derivation, then the expectation is that the latter sentence will be more difficult to perform (recall, respond to etc.). early experiments had encouraging results, later ones were positively discouraging (see fodor et al op. cit. and schlesinger, 1966). marshall and newcombe's analysis showed that aspects of the transformational grammar of the paralexic they studied had been interfered with through a lesion caused by a. bullet. this suggested quite startling confirmation for the psychological reality of linguistic constructs and came from an unexpected source; clearly, the transformational grammar of english which they used was not written with the aim of explaining linguistic disturbances. nevertheless, there remains something of a paradox in this work. it is that grammars formalize competence; they do not provide a model tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 a. traill of performance. yet in some cases they successfully predict performance complexity while failing in others. an attempt to get round this was made by fodor and garrett (fodor and garrett 1966) by claiming that the relationship between competence and performance is not as was previously thought, a direct one; rather it is abstract. but 'this move is quite unhelpful and seems to me to provide no useful exit from the dilemma. we are merely left with some experiments that worked and others that didn't. one wonders why any experiments worked at all. it is in the context of the foregoing that i would like to discuss the aphasia of w.n., a mother-tongue speaker of ndebele,, a south-eastern bantu language of the nguni group, spoken in southern rhodesia,, and except for minor differences the same as zulu. my purpose in exploring his linguistic impairment in transformational grammatical terms is not in order to make a direct contribution to the controversy over the relationship between competence and performance, although it is clear that the results unavoidably bear on this; instead, i am primarily interested in recording and analysing the case of an extraordinarily specific impairment in a language quite unlike english in its superficial aspects. history of the case w.n., an adult male aged 52, was referred to the university of the witwatersrand's department of speech and hearing therapy from baragwanath hospital, johannesburg, where he had undergone surgery for a head wound on the left side of his head1 sustained during an assault. on the schuell test he was diagnosed as suffering from severe expressive aphasia.2 this diagnosis is, of course, based on his performance in english, but it is supported by my data consisting only of his performance in ndebele. he is currently undergoing therapy in english and, at the time my data were collected, he had had a certain amount of therapy, also in english. the data the data on which the analysis is based were collected during four half-hour interviews over two successive weeks! each interview was taped and transcribed immediately thereafter. this enabled one to supply any contextual clues necessary for interpretation'. it should be pointed out that the two hours of interviewing time yielded a small number of utterances (105), which is, of course, understandable in view of w.n.'s expressive difficulty. however, since his performance did not vary in quality from one interview to the next, and since, as will become clear later, i was not concerned to analyse his impairment with respect to the whole language, but was concerned rather with one aspect of ndebele structure, i found the sample quite adequate for my purposes. in fact, the structural aspect of ndebele journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : the case of an ndebele speaking aphasic 51 in which i was interested, is so pervasive a feature in that language (or any bantu language) that it is unusual to find a sentence not manifesting this feature: this meant that nearly all w.n.'s utterances exemplified the difficulties he has with this feature. zulu-ndebele concordial structure while there are no authoritative sketches of zulu syntax in transformational generative (t.g.) terms, the theory is foi the most part explicit on the principles involved in accounting for concordial structure. this -does not mean that the theory dictates the solution in its ultimate details, but that it provides a set of constraints on the general form of the solution. put differently, it tells you where in the grammar various facts should be formalized. taking standard t. g. (the attribute "standard" has been introduced in order to distinguish earlier versions of the theory from recently proposed alternate models; chomsky, 1965, is regarded as a statement of standard t.g.) as the model for the following discussion, the components of a grammar and their inter-relationships can be represented as in figure 1 (adapted from fodor et al 1968). phrase s structure — y rules t ν lexicon τ a transformax tion rules deep structure s e m a n t i c c o m p o n e n t surface structure p h o n o l o g i c a l c o m p o n e n t semantic readings ^(meaning of sentence) phonetically interpreted ^strings (pronunciation of sentence) fig. 1 this grammar provides a specification of the meaning (semantic component) and pronunciation (phonological component) for each of the infinite number of grammatical strings enumerated by the syntactic component. the latter component consists of two sub-components, a base, in which phrase structure rules formalize those syntactic aspects of a sentence which affect the meaning of the sentence (categorial membership, e.g. χ is a noun, the χ is a noun phrase, and relational concepts, e.g. χ is the subject of the sentence), and a transformational component consisting of rules which produce surface structures through operations of deletion, substitution and adjunction on the strings provided by the base component. these transformational rules do not affect the meaning of the sentence; they provide the "syntactic trappings" (langacker 1968) of a language such as affixes, order of constituents, etc. once these rules have applied to produce a surface structure, the phonological component assigns a pronunciation to this structure. the lexicon consists of an unordered list of the lexical items in the language which are specified in terms of their phonological shape, their syntactic potential and their meaning. after the base sub-comtydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 a. traill ponent has operated to produce an abstract syntactic form, lexical items are. substituted from the lexicon at designated places, i t is the base structure with its lexical items which is the input to the semantic component. turning now to zulu, one finds a syntactic phenomenon which has traditionally been called the concordial system. this system can be characterized briefly and informally as a syntactic device which marks explicitly all those constituents in the sentence which are "governed by" a noun. furthermore, these explicit marks are phonologically "similar to" the prefix of the noun, whence the term alliterative concords in which "concord" refers to the syntactic relationship, and "alliterative" to the phonological one. (l)-(4) below illustrate this. 1. u.mu.ntu u.ya.hamb.a the person ' is going 2. i.si.tsha si.w.ile the dish has fallen 3. a.ma.hhashi a.mi a.ma.khulu ma.hle horses my big are beautiful ( m y b i g h o r s e s a r e b e a u t i f u l ) 4. a.ba.ntwana b.a.mi a.ba.khulu bahle children my big are beautiful ( m y b i g c h i l d r e n a r e b e a u t i f u l ) (full stops signify morpheme boundaries.) just what is meant by the term "alliterative concord" should be clear from these examples. as the governing noun changes, so do the prefixes on governed forms: possessives, adjectives, verbs, etc. traditional descriptions of this phenomenon have in the main simply listed the various noun prefixes and also the prefixes which appear on the governed forms. thus one finds lists of numbered noun prefixes possessive prefixes, adjectival prefixes, verb prefixes, etc. and a statement to the effect that possessives, etc. agree in number and class with the noun that governs them. thus, if the governing noun is class i then governed forms appear with the appropriate class i alliterative concords. in terms of figure 1, the account of this phenomenon is radically different. for example, in order to explain why concords appear where they do in sentences, we need to specify a set of syntactic conditions which formalize the notion of "governed constituent". these conditions are specified by the phrase structure rules of the base component. the explicit marking of the agreements is achieved through a transformational rule which marks governed forms with the class designation of the governing noun while at the same time capturing the alliterative aspect of the agreement by substituting the same phonological shape at appropriate positions in the sentence. in this treatment, therefore, concordial agreement is regarded as a "syntactic trapping" of zulu.' this follows from the fact that it is effected through transformational rules only. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : t h e case of an ndebele speaking aphasic 53 as illustration, consider the following highly simplified steps in the derivation of i.si.tsha si.w.ile (the dish has fallen) 1. deep structure representation in terms of the phrase structure rules: sentence(s) n o u n p h r a s e (np) n o u n (n) "+ ν — animate + concrete + singular i verb p h r a s e (vp) i verbal (vb) i + verb ( + v ) i : 2. lexical entry for i.si.tsha (dish) in the lexicon [.tsha] [ < c l a s s 7 > < — a n i m a t e ; » < + c o n c r e t e > ] [ m e a n i n g ] 3. lexical substitution in (1) np i ν .tsha + ν class 7 — animate + concrete + singular i vp i vb i +v i .w.ile 4. (3) is the input to the semantic component and has a meaning assigned to it. 5. (3) is also the input to the transformational component which performs the following operations on the sentence: 5.1. t-agreement. this transformation copies specified features of the governing noun onto governed forms, in this case the verb. s i n p i vp i 1 ν i 1 vb i 1 .tsha 1 + v + ν i class 7 w.ile — animate + concrete class 7 + singular _ : tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 a. traill 5.2. t-class segmentalization. the effect of this transformation is to convert a representation of the form x to < + y>" x j n this to < + y > x , in this < + y > :class 7 > . the output of this rule is: < + y > < + v > case < + y > is the feature . np i ν class 7 1 + affix j + .tsha ν class 7 — animate + concrete + singular vp i vb i + v class 7 "ί + affix j w.ile class 7 5.3. at this stage in the derivation a lexical pass which scans the sentence to determine whether any segment created by a transformational rule requires to be spelt out, will substitute the following lexical entry for each occurrence of ρ ? 3 8 ! 7 ] . l + affix j [si.] [ < + affix> < + singular>]. the effect of the lexical pass on the structure in (5.2) is: np i ν vp i vb i + v si. class 7 + affix + singular j + .tsha ν class 7 + animate + concrete + singular γ si. | class 7 i + affix l + singular 5.4. prothetic vowel spelling. for our purposes, at this stage of the derivation there are no further syntactic (transformational) rules to be applied. the structure in (5.3) therefore represents the input to phonological rules of which prothetic vowel spelling is the one required at this stage. its effect is to, copy the! vowel of the noun prefix before the consonant in the prefix. thus s i . u isi., or slightly more formally st.-> i s i . / + n , which says that si.· becomes isi. in the context of a journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : t h e case of an ndebele speaking aphasic 55 following noun.3 this completes the derivation, whose successive stages were: (i) [.tsha 1 w.ile (after lexical substitution in base) class 7 (ii) γ .tsha l class 7 j (hi) class 7 .tsha (iv) si.tsha (v) isi.tsha [ w.ile 1 (t-agreement) class 7 j class 7 .w.ile (class segmentalization) si.w.ile (lexical pass) si.w.ile (prothetic vowel). there are many complications facing anyone attempting to write rules which both simply and generally formalize all the facts of concordial agreement in a language like zulu, and the above illustration is by no means supported by an explicit account of these rules. but whatever these rules look like ultimately, it seems clear that at least the steps above will be required. in the discussion of w.n.'s performance, i shall maintain an informal approach to these rules while at the same time believing that they are in general outline correct. the structure of the noun consider the following examples of nouns produced by w.n., as single word responses to questions. when there is an error, the correct form appears in brackets. 1. i.hhashi horse 2. in.konyane calf 3. aba.ntu people 4. u.dokotela doctor 5. i.gwatsha rabbit (u.nogwatsha) 6. i.zulu zulu (isi.zulu) 7. um.gwatsha rabbit (u.nogwatsha) 8. n.kunzi bull (in.kunzi) 9. ma.thambo bones (ama.thambo) 10. ba.twana children (aba.ntwana) 11. .ntwana child (um.ntwana) 12. .komo cow (in.komo) 13. •godi hole (um.godi) 14. .gwatsha rabbit (u.nogwatsha) 15. .bisi milk (ubisi) (l)-(4) are all correct, both with regard to prefix and stem of the noun, (5)-(7) have correct stems and structurally perfectly good prefixes, except that they are the wrong prefixes for those stems. (8)-(10) have correct stems but only partially correct prefixes: each one lacks an obligatory initial vowel, as can be seen from an examination of the tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 a. traill correct forms. (11)-(15) have correct stems but no prefixes at all as can be seen from (5), (7) and (14), and (10) and (11), the same stem can appear with various prefix errors. these errors are for the most part readily interpretable in terms of the description of the preceding section. what is of particular interest, however, is that if one bases an error classification solely on these examples, then one finds that it is not always possible to identify a unique locus for an error. that is to say, a particular error could be the result of a breakdown of one or another rule and it is not possible to decide which rule it is. consider, now, the following "explanations" of the errors. from the first category ((5)-(7)), (6) is, in terms of what has preceded, the best example to discuss. the noun isi.zulu (the zulu language) has the following derivation: a i. s a a np ν np i ν α . ά np i ν np i ν .zulu 1 class 7 class 7 + affix .zulu [ class 7 γ s,. class 7 |_+affix .zulu class 7 .zulu the form which was produced, i.zulu, is a noun in zulu, but its class designation is "class 5", and its meaning is "heaven" or "sky". recall that in the lexicon a noun stem is specified as to pronunciation (p), class membership (c) and meaning (m). in this case, the pronunciation is correct, and the intended meaning was without doubt "zulu language". therefore, in the triple (p, c, m) the error must be in (c). bearing in mind that the same stem can manifest different prefix errors, we could suggest that (c) is unstable varying between and and presumably others. if it is substituted into the base with (c) as , the error could be explained. but there is another explanation which locates the error, not in the lexical entry for the stem but in the lexical entry for the prefix, which has the general form: [pronunciation] [class], if we allow the specification for [class] to be unstable here, the error would be/equally well accounted for. instead of steps (3)-(4) above, we would have (3')-(5')· a third possibility is that the locus of error lies in the specification of the pronunciation of the prefix in the lexicon. thus, instead of having [si.] [class 7], [li] [class 7] would occur. summarizing these three suggestions, the error could be located in (a) unstable (c) of the stem, (b) unstable (c) of the prefix, (c) wrong (p) of prefix. the only common feature these explanations possess is that they all locate the error in the lexicon and not in the syntax. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : t h e case of an ndebele speaking aphasic 57 3'. a λ , np i ν li .zulu class 7 class 7 + affix 4 ' . np i ν iii .zulu ((5') involves a rule not previously discussed). 5'. s a /\ np i ν a a i .zulu errors (8)-(10) are to be explained in terms of the failure to apply the transformation which segmentalizes the features < d e f > or < i n d e f > on nouns in positive contexts. the rule of prothetic vowel spelling is therefore blocked: there is no formative to be spelled. it is worth noting that when w.n. does spell out this vowel, it is always correct. when he fails to do so, therefore, he is not failing at a superficial phonological level, but at a deeper syntactic one. the reader should consult footnote 3 for a slightly more detailed discussion of these facts. (11)-(13) also have alternate explanations. firstly (c) of the lexical entries for -these stems could be missing which would mean that "class segmentalization" could not apply. secondly, it could be suggested that the entries for the stems were perfect, that "class segmentalization" did apply, but there were no lexical entries for the prefixes. the effect of this would be that step (2) above would be reached in the derivation of these nouns, but on the failure of any lexical material being substituted for the prefix (there being no entries for the prefixes), the segment [class x l would be deleted in accordance with the lexical seg[+affix j ment deletion convention (lsd) (rosenbaum 1967). thirdly, the error could result from conflicting (c) specifications on the stem and the prefix. notice that [si.] substitutes in step (3) above just because both it and the segment preceding the noun are identically specified [class 71. l+ affix j if the (c) of [si.] was, say , there would be conflict and no substitutions could take place, with the result that lsd would delete the segment preceding the noun, giving the forms in (11)-(15). in the summary, the errors (11)-(15) could be attributed to (a) nonexistent (c) on stem, (b) non-existent lexical entry for prefixes, (c) conflicting (c)'s on stem and prefix. again, all these suggestions locate the error in the lexicon. the "diagnosis" at this stage is that all errors excepting (8)-(10) are to be traced to instabilities or misinformation in the lexicon. it is interesting to note that what is essentially an "agrammatism" in traditional terms is not being located in the syntactic component of the tydskrif van diesuid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 a. traill description at all but in the lexical sub-component. this in fact reveals a central feature of transformational grammars: it is that the pairings of meaning and sound involve long and complex chains of rules and dependencies such that a decision to represent linguistic information in a particular form and at a particular point in the description will have repercussions throughout the derivation of a sentence. it follows from this that in the case of a linguistic pathology located at a specific point in the description, we can expect to find repercussions throughout the system, and if we pursue the eifect of the inadequate lexical entries for w.n.'s nouns and normal prefixes beyond the noun itself, we can expect to find far-reaching effects on other parts of sentence structure. hopefully, this may enable us to fix more accurately the location of his particular breakdown. the agreement transformation in 5.1. the effect of t-agreement was informally sketched: specified features of the governing noun are copied onto governed forms. this rule captures the notion that certain forms in the sentence agree syntactically with certain nouns. it is only in a subsequent rule in which the agreement is lexically "spelled out" that we formalize the further notion that the syntactic agreement is phonologically alliterative. a point worth emphasizing, is that this set of rules embodies a dependency, namely that certain constituents depend for their correct surface shape on a governing noun's syntactic feature composition. put differently, governed forms acquire their class prefixes; nouns have a class prefix as an inherent specification. in the light of the preceding discussion of the structure of w.n.'s nouns, it should come as no surprise to learn that his t-agreement produces unaeceptable surface structures. in terms of the grammatical model, however, it is the details of these derivations which are of interest. a most significant fact about the pattern of agreement errors, is that while there are a number of different types of error, they all reflect noun prefix governed prefix example correct form 1. correct a. correct b. incorrect c. zero u.nogwatsha u.hleli i.hhashi u.lele in.komo baleka / / i.hhashi li.lele in.komo i.ya.baleka 2. incorrect a. incorrect b. zero i.zulu i.nzima ma.phoyisa dinga isi.zulu si.nzima ama.phoyisa a.dinga 3. zero a. incorrect b. zero ntwana i.nye not in data um.ntwana mu.nye journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : t h e case of an ndebele speaking aphasic 59 the dependency mentioned above. the facts are summarized in the table below. there are no examples showing an incorrect or zero noun prefix matched with a correct governed prefix. this suggests very strongly that whatever else may be wrong with w.n.'s agreement rule, it always respects the dependency of governed forms on the governing noun; if the latter is inadequately or incorrectly specified, then the former will never show correct agreement. errors of the type (1c) (2b) (and the hypothetical (3b)) in the above table can be explained in a number of ways. firstly, t-agreement could have faired to copy the class feature onto a governed form, in which case the rule "class segmentalization" would fail, to apply with the result that no lexical substitution could take place, giving no governed prefix. secondly, it could not be held that t-agreement did apply, but class segmentalization did not, giving zero again. thirdly, both t-agreement and t-class segmentalization could have applied, but the lexical substitution of a prefix failed to take place (this type of failure was discussed above for noun-structure) with the resultant deletion of the segment f + ^ f f i x ] b y t h e l s d c o n v e n t i o n · what is clear from this discussion is that we are still no nearer establishing a unique locus for the errors. exactly why one should search for a unique locus derives, i think, from the feeling one gets from examining the errors, that w.n.'s concordial system does not work and one suspects intuitively that this disruption should be traceable to a unitary process in the grammar, rather than have it distributed over diverse rules. also, one can not shake the feeling that a unitary explanation will be a more parsimonious one, although i have no idea whatever about the relationship between occam's razor and an assailant's knife when the dissolution of language is being discussed. fortunately, there are a few crucial observations which suggest very strongly the interpretation that should be adopted. to say that w.n.'s concordial system does not work is misleading, for if one takes a careful look at what it is that is not correctly represented in his speech, one sees that there is evidence of a system of concordial prefixes but this system is inadequate with respect to normal zulu syntax because w.n.'s system is not alliterative.4 in other words, his system certainly allows for "agreements" (concord) between constituents; it even allows for these agreements to be pronounced in the form of prefixes. but, it does not require that the prefixes be alliterative. this is a crucial observation for we can now ask just where the notion "alliterative" is formalized in the grammar, and if this is done in one place, we shall have the locus of w.n.'s deficiency. a further observation which supports the contention that a system of agreement is in operation comes from the fact already noted that w.n. preserves the dependency of governed forms on a governing noun to the extent that we find no errors of the type incorrect governing noun prefix + correct governed tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 a. traill prefix. this means, that although it appears that the errors on nouns arise independently of the errors on governed forms, this is not in fact so; there is not complete freedom of choice for noun prefixes and governed prefixes. if this is so, a plausible suggestion would be that t-agreement always operates thus bringing governed forms into agreement with the governing noun. at a later stage the alliterative aspect of the agreement fails to materialize. the notion of alliteration is in fact uniquely located in the grammar: all concordial prefixes are alliterative simply because there is only one phonological entry for each class prefix. this means that the same phonological shape is substituted for each ["class x ] segment created [+affix j by t-class segmentalization. we need, therefore, recognize only one inadequacy in w.n.'s grammar in order to account for lack of alliteration and that is in the (c) specification for prefixes in the lexicon. the simplest solution would be to claim that he has no (c) at all but a list of entries for the prefixes specified as (p) [ < + affix> < + singular>]. the stage just prior to the substitution of an arbitrary prefix would then be ν class 7 + affix + singular (s) li. + affix + singular (l) .tsha (s) is the set of syntactic features and (l) the set of lexical features. the two sets are non-distinct (as opposed to being idenical (cf. chomsky 1965, p. 181)) thus allowing the substitution of < l i . > . this suggestion permits the substitution of any lexical entry specified γ + affix ] l + singular] to be substituted for any syntactic segment "class χ + affix + singular and any lexical entry specified " + affix "1 for any syntactic segment γ + afl l singular j class χ + affix . . _ singular this may be too powerful a rule since not all the possibilities it allows are found in the data. nevertheless, a sufficiently large number of the possibilities are attested for one to believe that the rule is correct in principle. in any event its power could quite easily be restricted.5 two further comments are required in order to complete the picture. firstly, under the present interpretation of w.n.'s deficiency, a completely successful marking of alliterative concordial agreement (1 (a) journal of the south african ogopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : t h e case of an ndebele speaking aphasic 61 in the table above) must be regarded as quite fortuitous. we are, therefore, not allowing for the possibility of the correct operation of the process being distorted only at a later stage, shall we say during the motor-phase of production of a sentence. we have, in effect, formalized his error in the grammar and are thus making the significant claim that his competence is impaired rather than claiming his error is due to an inadequacy in his performance mechanism. indeed, given that we know next to nothing about performance, it seems we have no alternative. we shall see later when examining w.n.'s performance on comprehension that this conclusion is at best a nuisance and at worst of dubious correctness. the second comment concerns the question of how it is that one finds a certain prefix appearing on the noun and a different one on the governed form. the explanation is that the convention governing lexical substitution does not allow more than one substitution at a time. presumably any segment [class x ] could be the first to undergo [ + affix j lexical substitution, but, presuming it is the noun prefix which is first affected, then it will be the case that the verb concord will be affected in a subsequent substitution. clearly, in the case of w.n.'s lexicon, the result of the second substitution need not be the same as the first. the explanation adopted so far accounts adequately for all types of error except those where no prefix materializes on either nouns or governed forms. all one needs in order to account for this category is to allow for the failure of lexical substitution of affixes; lsd does the rest. in case this proposal seems extremely ad hoc, let me point out that its effect would be to produce "telegraphese", and there is ample evidence of the phenomenon in the data. testing receptive control of concordial structure during interviews, w.n. showed no evidence at all of comprehension difficulties. but, owing to the extensiveness of concordial breakdown in speaking, coupled with the fact that concords are "syntactic trappings", and, therefore, do not contribute meaning as such, i felt it necessary to determine the extent to which concords were preserved in comprehension. an extremely simple but revealing test indicated quite clearly that w.n. was able to utilize the anaphoric properties of concords in order to arrive at the underlying meaning of a proposition. the test was based on the fact that, whereas in english pronominal forms such as "the one who is lying down" are multiply ambiguous (who or what does "the one" refer to?), in zulu one is able to narrow down the ambiguity of reference to a particular noun class. for example, the forms olulele, olele, elilele, elele, are all glossed in english as "the one who/which is lying down", but in zulu the first is "the class 11 which is lying down", the second "the class 1 or 3 . . .", tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 a. traill the third "the class 5 . . ." and the fourth "the class 9 . . .". w n was presented with various toys, the nouns corresponding to which fell into various classes. he was then asked to point to the one (ones) which was (were) lying down, following the man, eating the food etc a typical situation would be as follows: a duck (lying down) a'cow (lying down) and a tortoise (standing). he would be asked: khomba ehlele (point to the one which is lying down). the only correct response in this case is to point to the duck. if he had been asked: khomba elele, only the . cow would qualify. he performed successfully on all these tests. notice what he has to do in order to give the appropriate response firstly he has to identify the class designation of the pronominal prefix i.e. as class 5 or class 11 or class 1, etc. since this is an acquired prefix (governed form), he has secondly to identify an object in the situation whose noun is a class 5, 11, 1, etc. noun. let us say he has to "recover" the deleted noun which determined the form of the alliterative concord. having done this, he is then able to respond correctly. it cannot be argued that the extralinguistic context facilitates his response in such a test since this context is ambiguous with respect to objects lying down. successful performance here demands perfect control of the concordial "signals". this could be phrased differently as the reverse application of the transformations effecting agreement: the problem which now arises is that we have shown w.n.'s concordial processes to be intact in reception, but to be impaired in production. he thus shows perfect competence on the one hand and imperfect competence on the other.* yet competence in the technical sense is monolithic; it is neutral with respect to comprehension or production. how is this conceptual tangle to be resolved? there are three approaches to this issue. firstly, and i believe this is what a lot of linguists would claim, the tangle is the result of an illegitimate exercise in applied linguistics: linguistic theory was not intended for bizarre forms of language. secondly, one could adopt the view of weigl and bierwisch (weigl and bierwisch 1970) that all aphasia involves a breakdown in the performance mechanism; that the competence remains intact but is blocked. thirdly, along with whitaker (whitaker, 1969) one could argue that both competence and performance may be affected in aphasia and, in the case under discussion, the performance mechanism concerned with the speaking modality has broken down. the distinction between the last two views is that m the latter, impaired competence is countenanced, whereas in the former it is not. if w.n. had shown a defect in both the listening and speaking modalities whitaker would, in the absence of evidence to the contrary, be willing to diagnose his competence as being impaired. whitaker discusses the above three positions extensively and, rather than reiterate his lengthy arguments! here, i should merely like to note that the position he adopts must be seen in the context of his model for the representation of language in the brain and his views on the journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : t h e case of a n ndebele speaking aphasic 63 relevance of data from linguistic pathologies for assessing the explanatory power of a linguistic theory. but whether we adopt weigl and bierwisch's or whitaker's diagnosis in the present case, we are left with the same claim: w.n.'s deficit is a performance one; in whitaker's terms is would be attributed to the imperfect workings of the synthesizing "tracking" strategy in the mechanism for the speaking modality. exactly what this strategy is, and just how it would operate in the case of zulu concordial structure is something about which we can say nothing. this is, to say the least, an uncomfortable conclusion to arrive at, but in the context of current views on competence and performance it seems to be unavoidable. what then of the foregoing attempt to "formalise" w.n.'s nonalliterative concordial "system"? the positions i have just reviewed would doubtless regard this as wrong, and certainly if one accepts that competence is represented in the brain, that it is distinct from performance and, if one recalls the asymmetry between the listening and speaking modalities of w.n.'s performance, one is forced to agree with them. in order to legitimize the formalization i have attempted, it would have to be demonstrated that w.n.'s deficit at least cuts across all modalities, thus suggesting that competence has been affected (if i understand whitaker correctly, this would be his view)7. this follows from the assumption that competence is modality free. but this position reflects only a necessary condition for positing a competence deficit; it is not a sufficient condition simply because one cannot be sure, given an impairment in all modalities, that this is not perhaps a performance problem of a global nature. the global aphasia mentioned by weigl and bierwisch illustrates the undecidability of the problem: is this a total breakdown of the performance mechanism for all modalities with competence remaining intact; or is it a total destruction of competence with the consequence that the application of the performance mechanism is vacuous? (i wonder if the latter is a genuine possibility. if the domain of operation of the performance mechanism (competence) is removed, is there any reason why this mechanism should continue to exist?) owing to the general.limitations on our knowledge at present and on mine in particular, i do not propose to explore these questions any further. i should like, however, to make one observation concerning the type of information which would be necessary for the operation of whitaker's "tracking" strategy for that aspect of the speaking modality in zulu which involves making concordial agreement. if one accepts that w.n. suffers from a performance defect, then an examination of his errors in s.t.g. terms shows quite clearly that we need to recognize specifically syntactic "tracking" strategies which presumably must operate in real time prior'to and independently of phonological "tracking" strategies. the reason for maintaining this is that the syntactic tracking strategy which synthesizes the sentence at one level must, as the examples show, break down whenever a "segtydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 64 a. trai ment" categorized a s [ + affix] is "processed". w.n.'s difficulty cannot [class x j be relegated solely to inadequate operation of pronouncing strategies because his errors hinge on syntactic information. put differently, if by "performing a pronunciation synthesizing strategy" we mean the activation of a path through neuro-motor, myomotor and articulatory stages (liberman et al, 1967), it is not at all clear how a syntactic specification is carried through to these stages causing (?) a breakdown in operation. surely the proper functioning of this stage of production cannot be made to depend on syntactic information. put still differently, if an element [α β a b] is constantly (and variously) "mispronounced" in the first two 'phonemes' ab, and if ab also appears in other positions, but is never mispronounced there and, further, we discover that the first ab is in fact ab whereas the second is not, then • class x [ + affix obviously it is not just ab that is "difficult to pronounce"; rather it is ab when it is syntactically categorized as a class affix. an actual example is (abantu) babaleka ((the people) run away). w.n.'s performance interferes with the first ba, not the second. if performance strategies are, as is suggested above, sensitive to syntactic information presented in the competence formalization, it follows that this information must be duplicated exactly in the two components, once in competence and once in the tracking strategies. while i would hesitate to suggest that the brain hasn't enough room to accommodate this sort of duplication, i would venture no more than that it is surprisingly wasteful and is a direct consequence of claiming that competence has neurological correlates separate from performance. summary in summary, there are three points to mention: firstly, the attempt to analyse w.n.'s linguistic pathology in terms of a transformational generative grammar raised the problem of localizing the source of the defect (notice that the mere fact of being able to offer any localization in this grammar is an interesting result). secondly, the approach adopted, lead to the contradictory assertion that w.n.'s competence was impaired in production but not in comprehension. thirdly, this raised the vexing question of the relationship between' competence and performance and highlighted our ignorance of the operation of the latter. references bellugi, u. and brown, r. (1964) (eds.): t h e acquisition of language. m o n o graphs of the society for research 1 in child development, 29. chomsky, n. (1965): aspects of the theory of syntax. cambridge: m.i.t. press. journal of the south african ogopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) transformational g r a m m a r : the case of a n ndebele speaking aphasic 65 fodor, j. α., bever, t. g. and garrett, m . (1968): t h e development of psychological models for speech recognition. report n o . esd-tr-67-633 to the u.s.a.f. available from .the clearing house for federal, scientific and technical information. fodor, j. a. and garrett, m. (1966): some reflections on competence and performance in j. lyons and r. wales (eds.), psycholinguistic papers, e d i n b u r g h : e.u.p. jakobson, r. (1964): towards a linguistic typology of aphasic impairments. in α. v. s. de reuck and m . o ' c o n n o r (eds.) ciba foundation symposium on disorders of language. l o n d o n : j. & a. churchill ltd. jakobson, r. (1968): child language, aphasia and phonological universals. the h a g u e : mouton. langacker, r. w. (1968): language and its structure. new y o r k : harcourt, brace & world, inc. liberman, a. m., cooper, f. s., shankweiler, d. p. and studdert-kennedy, m . (1967): perception of the speech code. psychological review, 74, pp. 431 461. marshall, j. c. and newcombe, f . (1966): syntactic and semantic errors in paralexia. neuropsychologica, 4, pp. 169-176. mcneill, d. (1966): developmental psycholinguistics. in f. smith and g. miller (eds.), t h e genesis of language. cambridge: m.i.t. press. menyuk, p. (1969): the sentences children use. cambridge: m.i.t. press. rosenbaum, p. (1967): specification and utilisation of a transformational g r a m m a r . scientific report n o . 2 of the i b m corporation, new york. schlesinger, i. (1966): sentence structure and the reading process. t h e h a g u e : m o u t o n . weigl, e. and bierwisch, m . (1970): neuropsychology and linguistics: topics of common research. foundations of language, 6. whitaker, h. a. (1969): on the representation of language in the h u m a n brain. working papers, in phonetics, n o . 12. u c l a . footnotes 1. hospital records show that he suffered a fracture of the middle fossa of the skull, but i understand that this does not locate the fracture very precisely. beyond adding that he also had a right facial palsy, i am unfortunately unable to give a detailed medical diagnosis of the damage. 2. γη more detail, the diagnosis was aphasia with severe reduction of language in all modalities, complicated by sensori-motor complications. i am indebted to c. kell for supplying me with this diagnosis. as will become clear later, there is strong evidence to question the diagnosis and indeed i do not find this disagreement surprising; doubtless, the schuell test is confusing the aphasic impairment proper with the subject's bilingual inadequacies. 3. ε. b. van wyk has pointed out to me that this rule, as it stands, fails to reflect the fact that the appearance of what i have termed the prothetic vowel of the noun prefix is not a purely phonological matter. t h e presence or absence of a prothetic vowel reflects a semantic contrast (and therefore a deep syntactic choice) between, respectively, definite or indefinite n o u n s preceding or following a positive or negative verb, and a quantificational meaning something like "any x " where x is a noun following a negative verb. this means that the presence o r absence of a prothetic vowel in surface structure depends on certain deep syntactic features and the application (or not) of a segmentalising transformation; the final shape of this formative remains a phonological matter in zulu. t h e rule for prothetic vowel spelling presented in the text therefore requires a modification to reflect the syntactic conditions under which it may apply. in its present form, the rule does not reflect these conditions. 4. it is interesting to note that in the acquisition of their mother-tongue, zulu children pass through a stage of development which exactly parallels w.n.'s non-alliterative concordial system. 5. in this discussion, i am not concerned to write a rule which will completely account for the observed errors. my point is rather to isolate the general form of the disturbance. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 66 a. traill 6. w.n. actually shows more than just a breakdown of the alliterative concordial system. h e is unable, for example, to produce sentences containing embedded constructions, succeeding after much perseveration only to produce a co-ordinated paraphrase of such sentences. since this inability extends to right-branching structures as well, we could describe it more generally as a restriction on recursion in the grammar. 7. according to the schuell diagnosis, w.n. would thus show a disturbance in competence in english, but according to my test his receptive modality is intact for zulu. t h a t is his zulu competence is preserved. abstract an attempt is made to.explain certain expressive impairments in the speech of an ndebele speaking aphasic in terms of a transformational grammar. the impairment is eventually located in a part of the lexicon, but owing to the standard view of linguistic competence, this explanation poses problems which at present resist resolution. journal of the south african ogopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pierre de villiers pienaar m y r t l e l. a r o n , ph.d. (witwatersrand) head., department of speech pathology & audiology university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg summary the academic and professional background of the man w h o initiated and developed training programmes for s p e e c h therapists and audiologists in s o u t h africa is presented. the manner in w h i c h he d e v e l o p e d the profession, and pioneered work in p h o n e t i c s and linguistics is described, including p. de villiers pienaar's role in educational, academic, clinical and cultural institutions in s o u t h africa. opsomming die akademiese en professionele agtergrond van die man w a t die opleidingsprogramme vir spraakterapeute en o u d i o l o e in suid-afrika.ingestel en ontwikkel het, w o r d beskryf. die w y s e waarop h y die b e r o e p o n t w i k k e l het, sy pionierswerk o p die gebied van die fonetiek en linguistiek, a s o o k p. de villiers pienaar se rol in opvoedkundige, akademiese, kliniese en kulturele instansies in suid-afrika, w o r d bespreek. the study and practice of speech pathology and audiology in the republic of south africa has developed significantly since its introduction over a third of a century ago. pierre de villiers pienaar, with foresight and tenacity, was responsible for this development. at the time of his post-graduate studies in the late twenties at the university of utrecht, holland, and at the university of hamburg, germany, he recognized the possibilities of the rehabilitative application of facets emerging from the fields of phonetics and voice disorders. on his return to south africa, he presented during the period 1936-1938 the motivation to institute a training course in logopedics at the university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. his foresight in introducing this training programme and his awareness of the need to develop high academic and clinical standards demonstrate p. de v. pienaar's remarkable ability to view the profession of speech and hearing therapy in a clear perspective. his timing and fundamental appreciation of the overview of the wider issues involved and his constant scrutiny of the profession, have led to what is today a respected scientifically based profession whose practitioners enjoy good status. there are now opportunities for the further development of academic, clinical and research facets in this area. pierre de villiers pienaar was born in 1904 near potchefstroom in the transtydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 8 myrtle l. aron vaal and matriculated in the first class at the boys' high school in potchefstroom. in 1922 he attended the johannesburg teachers' training college and the university of the witwatersrand, obtaining a bachelor of arts in education and later an honours degree in the first class. in 1926 he graduated with the degree master of arts, with distinction, at the university of the witwatersrand. the latter two post-graduate degrees were in the area of phonetics and general linguistics, particularly afrikaans phonetics, this subject-matter also being the topic for his doctoral degree (ph.d.) earned at the university of hamburg, germany in 1929. the department of experimental phonetics at the university of hamburg, under the headship of professor g. panconcelli-calzia, had a profound influence on de villiers pienaar. not only did this department provide the first course in logopedics at a university in germany, but it was attended also by carl meinhof and g; kloeke who became foremost dialectologists and linguists, both adding considerably to the linguistic background of de villiers pienaar. during the years 1926-1927 and 1930-1932, he taught in primary and high schools in johannesburg. he was appointed lecturer in 1933 in phonetics in the department of african languages under professor c.m. doke at the university of the witwatersrand and remained at this institution for twenty-three years pioneering the work in phonetics and speech pathology. professor doke was held in high esteem for his work and publication of basic texts dealing with african languages. he also held a modern outlook concerning phonetics and the teaching of this subject, and strongly supported p. de v. pienaar's purpose in instituting a professional qualification for speech therapists. the first trained speech therapists completed their two-year diploma in logopedics at the end of 1939 at the university of the witwatersrand and received their clinical training at the speech, voice and hearing clinic at the same university. this clinic was established in 1936 by p. de v. pienaar with a view to training speech therapists. pierre de villiers pienaar was appointed senior lecturer in phonetics and logopedics in 1938, and in 1944 to the chair of the department of phonetics and logopedics. the speech, voice and hearing clinic flourished steadily and with his appointment to the chair he assumed the directorship of this clinic. professor pienaar left the university of the witwatersrand in 1957 and joined the university of south africa for two years as professor of afrikaans and nederlands linguistics. during this time he put forward proposals to create a professional degree course in logopedics at the university of pretoria. for eleven years (1959-1969) he was professor and head of the department of speech science, logopedics and audiology at the university of pretoria, and director of the speech, voice and hearing clinic, attached to that department. professor pienaar retired from this position at the end of 1969 but in order to benefit from his considerable experience, the university of pretoria continued to retain his services and he was appointed a temporary lecturer for the period 1970-1972. p. de villiers pienaar is now a professor emeritus at the university of pretoria and continues his avid interest in speech science, and in the area of diagnosis and rehabilitation of voice disorders. during the first decade after p. de v. pienaar had started the training facility for speech therapists, he continued to develop the programme methodically and with far reaching aims. he attracted lecturers from the universities of journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pierre de villiers pienaar 9 iowa and wichita, usa (the senior lecturer in logopedics, a.b. clemons and a lecturer in logopedics, h. wise) and from hamburg (lecturer in voice science, g.b. breckwoldt). not only did the original two-year diploma in logopedics become extended to a three-year diploma, but in 1948 professor pienaar was responsible for the introduction of the four-year professional degree course β .a. logopedics (now called b.a. in speech and hearing therapy at the university of the witwatersrand). the study of the field of audiology was formally introduced as a two-year major in the four-year curriculum at both universities in 1962, but prior to this, professor pienaar was well aware of the close relationship between hearing impairment, with its concomitant speech and language problems, and the general field of speech therapy. apart from introductory aspects concerning the ear and basic audiological procedures taught in the speech science course (phonetics i), he introduced a specific course dealing with aural rehabilitation as early as 1946. the contents of the degree course demonstrate the eclectic view he holds generally about the study of communication disorders. taking cognizance of the needs of the areas to be covered to adequately train speech therapists, p. de v. pienaar designed the curriculum offered for the four-year degree course as a non-elective one, except for a language in the first year of study, i.e. all subjects, with their practicums, are directly related to the aim of training speech therapists and audiologists. although some courses might have undergone modification in recent years since he introduced them, intrinsically the content of the course today at both universities carries the stamp of his design. students are introduced to the clinical situation from the first year of study (in structured observation sessions) and conduct therapy with a variety of cases under supervision from the second year of study. clinical practicums are held in both university clinics as well as in hospitals, primary schools, cerebral palsy schools and in special institutions. for curriculum purposes the minimum number of clock hours required by students for diagnostic and therapeutic work in the areas of speech pathology and audiology is 750. normally, the average student accrues nearly 1000 hours, a considerable amount for an undergraduate degree programme. the importance of supervised student clinical practicums has always been stressed by p. de v. pienaar and the manner in which he originally conceived how a speech and hearing clinic should function in a university department has without doubt paid substantial dividends. in addition to providing student clinical material, the two university clinics function as major facilities serving the communities of johannesburg and pretoria and the country areas. professor pienaar not only introduced and developed the training programme at the two universities, but has also been most active over the years in improving and maintaining the standards practised in the profession. he is the watchdog of what he has created and continues to pursue avenues and issues about any item which might have immediate or far reaching effect on the profession. he has always held meetings, called for discussions and has initiated correspondence with personnel here and overseas, all aimed to improve academic training, clinical standards, the creation of new posts and to report on what is being done in this country and overseas. on a carnegie fellowship in 1952, p. de v. pienaar spent an extensive period on research in speech tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 myrtle l. aron archives, logopedics and audiology in centres in europe, the united kingdom and the united states of america. on his return he incorporated many new facets in the academic and clinical work in the field. during 1962, and again in 1965, he received grants from the transoranje institute and the s.a. national council for the deaf to attend international conferences dealing with logopedics and audiology. at the time of these conferences he re-established and made new contacts and visited training institutions. professor pienaar was appointed honorary life president of the s.a. logopedic society nearly twenty years ago, the national body representing speech therapists and audiologists which has recently changed its name to the s.a. speech and hearing association. he exercises this role in a wide sense, drawing on outside organizations and professional persons in allied fields to take cognizance of the profession he has developed and nurtured. in 1966 de villiers pienaar instigated and convened an important round-table discussion on standards. this discussion was attended by representatives of several universities in south africa, the s.a. speech and hearing association and the supplementary health services committee of the s.a. medical and dental council, a statutory body maintaining the register for speech therapists and audiologists. the outcome of this round-table meeting resulted in far reaching implications. it led to a greater awareness of the profession and the role it plays in the rehabilitative service of any community in this country. it also led to modified and improved considerations taken up by the s.a. speech and hearing association. recently, this association introduced additional requirements for applicants seeking membership who have been trained in south africa or overseas, and in certain cases applicants now will have to undergo an examination before being admitted as full members. while demonstrating his awareness of the constant need to expand the field and improve on standards, p. de v. pienaar has also, since his earliest years in the profession, expended much effort on the establishment of posts for speech and hearing therapists in this country. he first persuaded the transvaal education department to employ speech therapists which they did from 1941. after this, also at his instigation, hospitals began creating more posts for therapists. scrutinizing the country as a whole he has always assessed specific developments taking place which might lead to rehabilitative or diagnostic services and therefore the employment of speech therapists and audiologists. he has always remained vigilant in his contacts with education departments throughout the country, medical and dental training departments, national health organizations and the defence departments. his contact with some of these institutions has also embraced the need to establish a base for'research to be conducted in the fields of normal and abnormal communication. today there are over 130 full-time posts in s.a. many therapists also conduct fulltime or part-time private practices. since the formal course in logopedics was introduced by p. de v. pienaar in the late thirties, over 380 speech and hearing therapists have graduated in this country. the number graduating in the profession will increase further when the university of cape town qualifies its first therapists after introducing a four-year professional degree course due to start in the very near future. this year the university of natal-westville introjourna of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pierre de villiers pienaar 1 1 duced a four-year professional degree in the field which will train indian theratte interests and activities of professor pienaar have been wider than his main nuroose in furthering the profession of speech pathology and audiology. his rademic work and teaching has revolved around phonetics and general linguistics and he has established speech science and language laboratories. he is also known as a pioneer in the study of experimental phonetics, particularly in relation to the phonemics of afrikaans, at both the universities of pretoria and the w i t w a t e r s r a n d . he has interested himself over many years w i t h t h e ohonetics of bushman dialects, diachronic linguistics in connection with the phonological structure of afrikaans, and the linguistic aspects of speech disorganization. concerning lexicological work he has collaborated with professors m.s.b. kritzinger and f.j. labuschagne on the verklarende afrikaanse woordeboek of w h i c h the 6th enlarged edition has just appeared.9 the research he has conducted in the field of afrikaans phonetics earned him the erepenning" award of the suid-afrikaanse akademie vir wetenskap en kuns in 1963. still today in his retirement professor pienaar is engaged in this work as well as establishing archives in linguistics and speech science in south africa. he has been appointed head of the speech archives sponsored by the department of national education and the human sciences research council. among his students who majored in phonetics, three have become heads of university departments of phonetics or african languages in south africa (l.w. lanham, d.t. cole and e.o.j. westphal). the membership or alliance pierre de v. pienaar holds with various bodies is extensive and reflects his wide interest within the area of normal and disordered human communication. his membership on committees or appointments to bodies include: the language advisory committee of the s.a. broadcasting corporation, from 1957-1968; the advisory committee for language laboratories of the government department of education, arts and science; the film board of the republic of south africa since its inception; the representative of the department of national education on the board of the transoranje institute for special education; the editorial board of folia phoniatrica, the journal published by the international logopedic and phoniatric society; the committee for controlling speech examinations and moderator for the university of south africa since its inception up until 1971; the commission appointed by the department of health to investigate the use and abuse of hearing aids in south africa. in addition to being a member of learned societies both national and international, he has also served for many years on clinical and technical committees of the s.a. national council for the deaf, the transoranje school for partially hearing children, the national committee for noise control and the s.a. speech and hearing association. p. de v. pienaar was an elected member of the „maatschappij der nederlandsche letterkunde te leiden." over many years he has given considerable advice and direction to professional and academic institutions in this country who turn to his experienced and objective counsel. the interests of pierre de v. pienaar also extend to wider cultural areas. he has published a novel1 0 and collections of short s t o r i e s 1 1 , 1 2 ' 1 3 , is editor and tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 12 myrtle l. aron a contributor to the „kultuurgeskiedenis van die afrikaner" (three volumes)15 and was a founder member of die handhawersband, an afrikaans cultural association, and became the first editor of die handhawer. he has a keen interest in music and is well known for his deep knowledge about opera and singers, being particularly concerned with the art of singing. speech therapists in this country have for many years associated p. de v. pienaar with his work in voice and its disorders. his diagnostic acumen and therapeutic skill with voice disordered cases have earned the respect of several ent specialists who are increasingly referring cases of this nature to him and to other speech therapists. in his retirement he is continuing to work in this area, again pursuing developmental work which has been comparatively neglected compared to other disorders of communication. the dedication of this issue of the journal to pierre de villiers pienaar reflects, in small measure, the large tribute which speech therapists and audiologists in ' this country have for the man who has initiated their profession and developed it with such purpose and tenacity. he pioneered the field and its academic training. p. de v. pienaar's.steadfast application concerning the expansion and improvement of clinical, academic and research facilities will always be remembered with admiration and respect. he will always be known, not only as a phonetician and a linguist, but the father of speech therapy and audiology in south africa. we all wish him good health in his retirement and look forward to the fruits of the work he is now undertaking. list of publications of pierre de villiers pienaar 1. with t.h. le roux (1927): afrikaanse fonetiek. j.c. juta, johannesburg. 2. (1930): die fonoposotie in die fonotopie van afrikaanse afsluitings en vernouingsklanke binne die spraakmolekuul swets en zeitlinger, amsterdam. 3. (1939): praat u beskaaf. voortrekkerpers, johannesburg. 4. (1947): die afrikaanse spreektaal van schaik, pretoria. 5. (1945, 4th edition 1963): uitspraakwoordeboek van afrikaans. van schaik, pretoria. 6. with a.g. hooper (1948): an afrikaans-english phonetic reader. rand university press, johannesburg. 7. (1951): chapter entitled "speech disorganization", in social medicine, ed., e.h. cluver. central news agency, ltd., johannesburg. / 8. (1966): chapter entitled "speech pathology in south africa": in speech pathology, eds., rieber, r.w. and brubaker, r.s. north holland publishing company, amsterdam. 9. with kritzinger, m.s.b. and labuschagne, f.j. (1973): afrikaanse verklarende woordeboek. van schaik, pretoria. literary works ; 10. (1928): skakels van die ketting. de bus$y, holland. 11. (1934y.ruth e.a. kortverhale. nasionale pers, johannesburg. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pierre de villiers pienaar 1 3 π 936)· magte e.a. kortverhale. nasionale pers, johannesburg. i3 (1940): die oorlogskind e.a. kortverhale. nasionale pers. johannesburg. cultural works t/i π 951v opera en sanger. afrikaanse pers, johannesburg. ! s e d i t o r with van den heever, c.m. (1945-1947-1950): kultuurgeskiedenis van die afrikaner. three volumes. nasionale pers, johannesburg. (1968): as editor, abridged and revised edition of kultuurgeskiedenis van die a frikaner, nasionale pers, johannesburg. ζβΐίίίαη & o n p r o p e r t y b r o k e r s p.o. box 5758, johannesburg tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) apraxia dysarthria η. reef, m.b., b.ch. (rand), m.r.c.p.e., m.r.c.p.. (lond.) p a r t ι the diagnosis of apraxic dysarthria still puzzles many speech therapists, and some even doubt its existence despite the fact that it is firmly established in the literature, and at least one standard textbook on speech disorders devotes a whole chapter to the condition. this paper attempts to explain the condition, how it comes about, its clinical manifestations, and possible methods of treatment. consider the following case: john i. is aged seven years. he was born after a normal full term pregnancy, but labour was prolonged and forceps were applied towards the end of the second stage. he appeared to be a healthy infant and developed normally. he sat at five months, crawled at eight months, and walked at thirteen months. he said his first words at fourteen months which were "mamma" and "nanna". thereafter, his vocabulary slowly increased, and he began to use two to three word sentences only, at the age of three years. from an early age it was noticed that his speech was not clear and this -has persisted until now. in all other respects he is quite normal. his parents and siblings understand his speech, but strangers have great difficulty in doing so. he started going to school one year ago and there his speech disorder has become a source of embarrassment. the children tease him and ridicule his "baby talk". as a result, he has become sullen and withdrawn. he can be persuaded to go to school with great difficulty. he has had speech therapy since the age of five years, with little benefit. the family history background is quite normal. there is no suggestion of any emotional disturbance. the mother, father and two siblings are right-handed, and all speak normally. speech examination: this revealed that child's speech is at times almost unintelligible. he has little trouble with vowel sounds or short words, but long words and consonants cause him great difficulty. he regularly mispronounces the "k", "t", "w", "s", "d", and "g" sounds. there are many substitutions and omissions, especially of final sounds. when persuaded to speak very slowly, speech becomes much clearer. he can repeat individual sounds and words quite well. there was no evidence of dysphasia and intelligence as tested on the south african individual scale was above average. physical examination: this revealed no significant abnormalities, except that the reflexes in the right half of the body were slightly brisker than those on the left, and the right lower limb was slightly underdeveloped. he is right-handed. there was no evidence of any dysfunction in the lips, tongue, palate or larynx. his reading, writing, and spelling were within normal limits. into which diagnostic category can this child be placed? he shows evidence of a very mild left cerebral (dominant) hemisphere lesion, but this has not produced any physical defect in the function of the lips, tongue, palate and larynx. however, when he speaks a serious disorder tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.14, nr. 1: sept. 6 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 38 η. reef appears. his higher speech centres are intact and there is no evidence of any weakness, ataxia, or sensory loss in the lower organs of speech, and no structural defect or hearing loss. psychogenic factors have been excluded. where then does the defect lie? he has been previously diagnosed as suffering from dyslalia, but has not responded to suitable therapy. the latter diagnosis thus appears to be wrong, for the following reasons: 1. there is evidence of slight brain damage (the possible cause being a forceps delivery). 2. the condition is far too severe and persistent. 3. therapy for dyslalia has been unsuccessful. 4. there is no apparent cause for dyslalia. it is thus suggested that the correct diagnosis is apraxic dysarthria. a consideration of apraxic dysarthria when the volume of sound and symbolic content of speech is normal, but the articulation and enunciation of the individual words and phrases are distorted, a patient is said to be suffering from dysarthria. apraxia implies that the motor disorder is due to a lesion in the highest motor centres of the cerebral cortex where voluntary movements are initiated, planned and synthesised, so that the correct result is obtained in the peripheral motor organ. the correct organisation of movements is called praxis, and a disturbance of this function is termed apraxia. it has nothing to do with the strength of muscle contraction, co-ordination or sensation. there are many definitions of apraxia, but most comprehensive is that given by worster-drought: "the inability, as the result of an organic brain lesion, to execute familiar, purposive, more or less automatic movements, when there is neither motor paralysis, sensory disturbance, ataxia, or any intellectual impairment." it is as if the patient retains the will to perform the act, and retains the neurological apparatus capable of performing the movement, but between the two there is a gap which he is unable to bridge. the patient knows what he wants to do but cannot do it. the essential feature of the condition is that voluntary movements are affected so that the same movement which cannot be carried out to command, or voluntarily, may still be carried out reflexly. e.g. a patient may not be able to protrude his tongue when asked to do so, but a moment later he may carry out this movement to lick his dry lips. similarly, a devout christian may not be able to make the sign of the cross on request, but will do so reflexly on entering a church. the movements of speech are carried out in an almost automatic manner, and must toe considered as the most highly skilled motor patterns that the body possesses. how these and other skilled movements, for example those of the hands, are carried out is still unknown but a study of apraxia does shed some light on the subject. electrical stimulation of the motor cortex in the frontal lobe or the motor centres in the extra-pyramidal system produces crude movements. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) apraxia dysarthria 39 a lesion in the motor cortex will produce loss of movements or weakness for both voluntary and reflex movements. this is not apraxia. the lesion causing apraxia must be situated at a higher level, or in a higher motor centre. this higher level or centre is not a small discrete structure or isolated portion of the brain, but involves probably most of the cortex of both hemispheres. apraxia may be produced by lesions in either hemisphere and in almost any region. the most likely site for such a lesion is, however, anterior to the motor cortex in the dominant hemisphere. how are skilled movements organised in the cerebral cortex? if we desire to initiate or execute a movement, then this must be translated into some method whereby the motor cortex is stimulated. this transition from a psychological state to physical action in the motor cortex was always assumed to be" achieved by means of transcortical fibres. in other words, messages were conducted from areas of the brain said to be concerned only with psychological mechanisms to regions where they could be translated into physical action. we now know that this is probably not so, since these transcortical fibres can be severed with little effect on voluntary movements. another mechanism therefore has to be postulated, and we now believe that these stimuli descend to lower co-ordinating centres in the brainstem, e.g. the reticular formation, and then ascend again to stimulate the motor cortex. the acquisition or learning of a skilled movement in childhood starts as an almost unconscious process by first bringing into play all the sensory faculties, so as to see, feel and even "hear" the movement. these sensory experiences are then used to try and reproduce a similar motor phenomenon. by constant repetition kinetic engrams or "memories of movement" are stored in the neuronal circuits. these engrams then become part of our permanent motor repertoire and we can recall them whenever necessary. if we store faulty engrams, or cannot properly recall them, then we will have imperfect movements. it is these engrams which in turn stimulate the motor cortex. the process of carrying out a skilled movement can be summarised as follows: 1. the idea of the movement is formulated spontaneously, or in response to an external command. 2. correct psychic planning of the action. 3. correct mobilisation of the kinetic engrams. 4. stimulation of the motor cortex. if the first step is defective, this leads to a paucity or absence of voluntary movements. perhaps the catatonic form of schizophrenia is representative of this type of disorder. a disturbance in the second and third steps leads to apraxia. the following discussion on the various types of apraxia is based on the classification proposed by liepmann. ideational apraxia. if there is incorrect psychic planning of. the action, then the patient is said to suffer from ideational apraxia. the basic defect is a disorder of the conception of the required movement. patients know and understand each individual part of the movement, but mistakes tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) η. reef occur. they cannot reproduce them in the correct sequence and at the proper time. the usually quoted example of this condition is the patient who cannot light a. cigarette. he commits surprising errors without being aware of them. he will take out a match, place it in his mouth, and rub the cigarette against the matchbox. this type of apraxia will only affect a complex .movement. the patient, can still carry out simple movements when requested to do so, e.g. a symbolic gesture. the kinetic engrams are still intact, but they are mobilised incorrectly. idiokinetic apraxia. the patient knows what he should do and how he should do it, but cannot mobilise the necessary apparatus of action. the engrams are there, but he cannot voluntarily set them in motion. then suddenly he will do so in a reflex or automatic action. our earlier example of the person who could not make the sign of the cross falls into this group. another example is that of the patient who cannot wave goodbye on request, but will do so when the doctor leaves the ward, or he cannot point on demand to a specific object, but can do so if he suddenly develops the need for it. this type of .apraxia mainly affects symbolic gestures, and it is mainly the intentional or volitional use of the movement which is. disturbed. motor apraxia. in this type of apraxia it is postulated that there is a fault in the engrams themselves. they are either absent or faulty. if a patient, through some disease process, loses his motor engrams, then he will behave as if he is carrying out that movement for the first time. he may lose his engrams for only one specific function, e.g. an expert pianist will sit down at the piano and be. quite unable to play (instrumental amnesia). a patient may lose the ability to write (agraphia), or play some game at which he was previously skilled. if the engrams are there, but they are faulty, the movements are not lost, but they are performed incorrectly. bucco-linguo-facial apraxia belongs to this group. if the engrams for movement of the lips, tongue and palate are lost completely, then the patient becomes anarthric. if they are faulty, then a dysarthria will result. from the above discussion it can now be postulated that an apraxic dysarthria may arise in one of two ways: 1. the kinetic engrams may be at fault. 2. they may be incorrectly or incompletely mobilised. in this way the inability to reproduce certain sounds and the omissions and substitutions which form the basic symptoms of an apraxic dysarthria may be explained. translated into speech terms consonants, diphthongs and vowels are affected, in this order. this may be explained on the basis of the relative complexity of the sounds involved, and therefore on the complexity of the motor engrams involved. the more complex engrams would be affected first, and therefore consonants would be disturbed first. in very severe cases all three may be affected. the literature on the subject of apraxia is vast, and not all authors agree on the same classification. many, special types of apraxia have been described, of which one is of some clinical importance. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) apraxia dysarthria 41 dressing apraxia. a highly specific disorder in which the patient loses the ability to dress himself. basically, this is due to a disturbance of body image and a failure to pay due attention to both the left and right, halves of the body and garment concerned. expressive aphasia could, according to. -our definition of apraxia, be classified as such a type of disorder. indeed, some authors follow this practice. this is incorrect, since the term apraxia refers only to motor functions,' and aphasia to the higher psychological or symbolic' functions of spee;h. the distinction, however, is probably a very fine one, and the same process may well be involved in both types of disorder. it is certainly no accident that dysarthria is seen so frequently in aphasic patients. part 2 by definition the disorder of apraxia dysarthria is due to an organic brain lesion and the patients who come for therapy do not suffer from any of the progressive diseases which lead to total incapacity or death. in practice the exact cause is of no great importance as the patient is usually seen long after he has sustained his brain injury and the neurologist and therapists are asked to diagnose and treat the residual disabilities. the pathological processes which may cause an apraxic dysarthria vary widely. in children the commonest causes are developmental disorders, . birth trauma, infectious diseases, and head injuries sustained during infancy. in adults caused factors are cerebro-vascular accidents and head injuries. • all these disorders may cause widespread damage so that the apraxic dysarthria is associated with other obvious neurological disabilities. such cases do not generally cause any diagnostic difficulties. even if the true nature of speech disorder is not recognised, the organic factor will be recognised. the only difficult group of cases are those patients where the speech defect appears as an isolated phenomenon set against a background of an otherwise apparently normally functioning nervous system. the clinical picture is nearly always the same. the patient, usually a child, is brought by the parents because his speech is not clear and it has become a social embarrassment. there has frequently been a delay in the development of speech, but this is seldom severe. the mother reports that the child understands speech very well and that within the immediate family circle he can make himself understood quite adequately. strangers, however, have great difficulty in understanding the child. his vocabulary may be very good. simple words are often pronounced quite clearly but multi-syllable words cause most difficulty. when the child talks quickly the condition becomes more marked. sometimes a word is pronounced clearly and a few sentences later the same word is unintelligible. as may be expected consonants are far more severely affected than vowels or diphthongs. the consonants are misplaced, transposed or omitted. substitutions are frequent and often the final sound of a word is left out. the following are typical examples: tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 42 η. reef ι . sang for swing. 2. otomo for tomato. 3. sutter for supper. 4. enil for pencil. 5. yell for yellow. 6. poto for potato. 7. bobo for bottle. it is often surprising how the older child with this type of disorder does not recognise or hear his own bad speech—as if he had an auditory imperception for his own speech. he will hopelessly mispronounce a word he has just heard and appear satisfied with his effort. yet it will be easy to establish that his hearing and interpretation of sounds are normal. such speech defects are normal in young infants when they are learning to talk. parents will often imitate them and use the same mispronunciation when talking to the child. when the child is young this type of speech is considered "cute". when such speech persists and becomes a firmly established pattern, then anxiety results and help is sought. firstly an attempt should be made to establish the organic nature of the disorder. this is done by searching for evidence of a possible neurological cause which may have arisen in the birth process or early development of the child. in some cases even although there does not appear to be any obvious neurological evidence of brain damage, the history may suggest possible brain damage, e.g. delayed milestones, unexplained convulsions or behaviour disorder. speech examination. this consists of an examination of the peripheral organs of speech to establish that there is normal function for all voluntary and involuntary movements, other than speech. 1. establish that the tongue and lips function normally for smiling, kissing, "pulling faces", licking the lips, protrusion and diadokinesis of the tongue. 2. test palate by examining the child's ability to swallow and by examining palatal elevation on stimulation or saying "ah". 3. assess co-ordination of respiration and phonation by watching and listening. the findings here will of course depend entirely on the site and degree of brain damage. from this point of view we may divide our patients into two groups: 1. those with very slight brain damage. there may be no abnormal physical signs, although this is rare. a very careful search will nearly always reveal some abnormality, e.g., excessive clumsiness, slight wasting on one side with a change in reflexes, involuntary movements which are very slight, unestablished laterality. 2. those with obvious signs of brain damage, e.g. the adult who has recently had a cerebro-vascular accident resulting in a hemiplegia with a unilateral facial weakness and no dysphasia. speech may be grossly dysarthric and obviously "the facial weakness alone cannot be responsible. this group can also include the child who has a spastic quadriparesis. the cranial nerves appear to function adequately on formal examination, yet speech is very indistinct. with these patients it is sometimes difficult to journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) apraxia dysarthria 43 decide whether the defect in neuro-muscular control of the lips, tongue and palate is responsible for the dysarthria or whether the lesion is situated at a higher level causing an apraxia. if a patient is old enough, a search should be made for disorders of perception, reading and writing as these often co-exist with an apraxic condition. where indicated a detailed assessment of intellectual functions should be carried out. finally, if clinical methods have failed to provide evidence of an organic lesion then one may have to resort to special investigations, such as an eeg examination. this may be more helpful and will sometimes reveal evidence' of cerebral dysfunction when all other methods have failed. differential diagnosis it is evident that apraxic dysarthria must be distinguished from dyslalia and developmental dysarthria. the latter condition can be excluded by careful local examination when the cause of the dysarthria will immediately become apparent. differentiating dyslalia from apraxic dysarthria gives rise to greater difficulty. most cases of apraxic dysarthria initially seem to be diagnosed as dyslalia and in this respect a question of terminology arises. dyslalia has really almost the same meaning as dysarthria. speech therapists have applied the term dyslalia to a group of children who articulate incorrectly due to one of the following five causes: 1. persistence of faulty habits of articulation. 2. imitation of faulty patterns of articulation. 3. the influence of defects of vision on articulation. 4. mental defect. 5. environmental and psychogenic factors. in fact, only the first of these five groups give rise to diagnostic difficulty. the other four can be readily excluded with a careful history and examination. if the dysarthria is severe and persistent, then it is hardly likely to be due to faulty habits. if the dysarthria is mild then difficulty may arise. the most important aid in making the correct diagnosis is the finding of an organic neurological disturbance. once this has been established then dyslalia or any other functional disturbance can be excluded. summary the nature of apraxic dysarthria is defined and a short description of its manifestations has been given. the relationship between apraxia and apraxic dysarthria is discussed. clinical manifestations and the diagnosis of the disorder are described in detail. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 14, nr. 1: sept. 196 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 44 η. reef opsomming die aard van apraktiese-disartrie word gedefineer en 'n kort beskrywing van die manifestasie van die toestand word gegee. die verhouding tussen apraksie en apraktiese-disartrie word bespreek. diagnose en kliniese manifestasie van die afwyking word meer volledig beskryf. references 1. m o r l e y , μ . e. ( 1 9 6 5 ) : the development and disorders of speech in childhood. edinburgh and london: e. & s. livingstone, ltd. 2. nathan, p. w. ( 1 9 4 7 ) b r a i n , 70, 449. ' ' „ t 3. worster-drought, c. (1952): diagnosis of nervous disorders. london: edward arnold. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 14, no. 1: september 1967 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society page een brain damage in children by dr. ruth clark perhaps one of the most neglected areas in the whole field of brain damage is one in which the damage is minimal and diffuse. more and more we are beginning to realize that slight brain damage may be the cause of some:types of learning disabilities, behavior problems and even juvenile delinquency. when we talk about brain-injured children, much of the nomenclature is confused and ambiguous. perhaps the following definitions will help clarify some of the terms used: , 1. congenital is a time-of-occurence concept. it means that the condition was present at the time of birth. it has nothing to do with heredity. 2. heredity is a causal concept and indicates that the. present characteristics are a result of the condition of the germ plasm. 3. endogenous is usually used in relationship to mental retardation and indicates that the condition is hereditary. 4. exogenous usually refers to the type of mental retardation that is not from heredity but from injury to the organism during the pre-natal, neo-natal or postnatal period. some authors use the term to indicate brain injury that is acquired but does not necessarily make the child mentally deficient. 5. mental deficiency can be, primary, i.e. of the endogenous type, or secondary, the exogenous type and merely means that the individual is intellectually functioning below the average. brain injury can cause mental deficiency. 6. amentia means without or lacking mentality and therefore, refers to mental deficiency. this term is further divided into: primary amentia —1 one whose familial background indicates a history of hereditary deficiency, and " secondary amentia — an acquired condition. 7. cerebral palsy is a result of brain injury. it is not a disease but aj syndrome, in which neuro-muscular dysfunction is the| outstanding symptom. cerebral palsy is a term applied to a disturbance of motor function resulting from damage to the brain before, during or after birth. it connotes a group of conditions. these conditions are very different, and consequently, the treatment of the different types of cerebral palsy requires totally different techniques. 8. dysphasia and aphasia are language disturbances. they are disorders of symbolic formulation and expression, and are a result of damage or lack of maturation of the associational areas of the brain. the dysphasias .failure .to properly, use the word "pencil" is notdue to a motor disability but rather, to a failure an associating ;and integrating three different aspects of the."pencil'.', .that is, the pencil as: (1) a real object of senses of sight, sound, smell, touch, etc.; (2) the sound of the word "pencil" which by convention, has come to represent the real object and (3) the neuro-muscular reaction involved in uttering the spoken word " pencil". since reading, writing, understanding and use of objects are associated with language, these skills may also be deficient when an individual suffers disturbances in the associational areas. aphasia and dysphasia like cerebral palsyare not diseases themselves; they are a symptom of a diseased process and are not essentially a speech problem, but rather a language disorder. since deaf children have language problems, aphasic children are frequently confused with deaf children. with the deaf, sounds are not heard; with the aphasic, sounds are heard but they are not translated into meaning. 9. emotional disorders are many times confused with mental deficiency, deafness and aphasia. while there is always, "or practically always, a psychological component accompanying the above-mentioned conditions, there can be emotional disturbances without brain damage of any sort. if the above, terms are kept clearly in mind and appropriate diagnostic instruments used to determine the real difficulty, .with children presented at speech clinics,·, therapy much , more appropriate for the individual case can be; instigated. while present diagnostic instruments are not refined enough in many cases to enable us to. see the complete and accurate picture of children presented for therapy, they give us a "better picture than we could otherwise obtain, and if we are aware of their limitations, we can more easily avoid the pitfalls that we might encounter if we were not forewarned. many children suffering from the conditions referred to above are recognized and usually some type of provision is made in'' their training for their particular handicap. -however, the child who has a very slight, non-localized brain injury frequently is not recognized and yet, he may have certain difficulties for which special allowances should be made. he with the more easily-recognized brain-injured child, is -apt to have perceptional,'' thinking and behavior difficulties. this minimal, diffuse brain injury results from many causes such as the mother running a high temperature before the child's birth, oxygen starvation before or during or after birth, severe childhood diseases, and almost any condition that will cause cerebral palsy or secondary amentia. characteristics of brain injury any serious student in this area realizes that there is a great deal of confusion regarding the characteristics r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) page eight journal of the south african logopedic society of brain-injured children. probably one reason is that the literature pertaining to the field emanates from many different areas and frames of reference. among personnel contributing to the literature are speech pathologists, students of mental deficiency, specialists from several areas in psychology and medicine, as well as educators. in spite of the confusions in the field, the following characteristics-of brain injury are fairly well accepted by the different writers: 1. brain lesions produce similar behavior regardless of the location of the lesions. 2. rigidity and perseveration — this refers to the inability to change easily from one "mental set" to another and the tendency to continue an acceptable performance. 3. perceptual difficulties — perception is the mental process which gives particular meaning and significance to a given sensation and therefore, acts as a preliminary to thinking. one basic characteristic of perceiving is that a perception is made as a whole, "all at once and nothing first". this power of integration is dependent upon an intact nervous system. brain injured children are frequently so attracted to minute details that they disregard the conceptual concept as a whole. some writers refer to this as forced responsiveness. such a reaction is exemplified by the child who is so attracted by a pretty button on a dress that he reacts to this unessential factor alone, disregarding the. essential factors in his environment. sometimes labeled as "inattentiveness," it is really the exact opposite: a complete attentiveness (to something which the normal person would not notice, and therefore an inattentiveness to everything else). 4. thinking disorders — clinical observations of these children have yielded evidence of peculiarities in thinking reasoning, and concept formation deviation markedly from the normal. the brain-injured child is easily prone to give responses which are uncommon, far-fetched, and often peculiar. he is attracted more easily by unessential and accidental details, relationships between object and picture are drawn vaguely, situations are imagined which extend beyond the present situation. 5. behavior disorders and disinhibition: some studies have shown brain-injured children to be erratic, uncoordinated, uncontrolled, uninhibited and socially unaccepted. they are apt to have extreme mood swings and while laughing and playing, burst into explosive crying when confronted with a difficulty. diagnosis the difficulties of properly diagnosing a child having minimal diffuse brain damage are many. frequently, the difficulty lies deep in the cerebrum and does not show up on an electro-encephalograph recording or in a neurological test. many times the only way a child can be classified as having minimal diffuse brain damage is by ruling out every other possibility. the problems of diagnosis are too manifold to be discussed nere. but it is this difficulty which has caused so many children who are probably slightly brain-injured to be classified as hard of hearing, mentally defective, problem children or psychotic. these children then receive therapy that is not structured for their true needs. diagnosis of the minimally brain-injured is one of the most challenging problems in the field today. when confronted with a child having perceptual thinking or behavior problems for which we can find no etiology nor reason, we might well consider the possibility of diffuse brain damage of a minimal amount. in that case different therapeutic procedures will be used than would be if we were working for example, with a behavior problem without any organic background or with a mentally retarded child of the familial type. therapy for the brain-injured child therapy is based upon controlling and helping the child to control the perceptual, conceptual and behaviorial problems which clinical observation and research have indicated to be peculiar to the braininjured child. the stimuli of an ordinary environment are simplified so that the child can build up a threshold of endurance to them. a few points for the speech therapist to keep in mind when working with braininjured children follow: 1. keep environmental stimuli reduced. (a) few, if any, pictures or decorations on walls. (b) quiet room. (c) all materials put away that are not in use. (d) furniture placed so as to give sense of space. 2. insofar as possible, use concrete objects and concrete words when beginning therapy. (a) dean is an example: in teaching him the names of knife, fork,, and spoon, the clinician had little success when she used pictures; as soon as she took him over to the dining room for bis speech lesson and had him use actual utensils, learning progressed. 3. keep materials simple. (a) pictures which contain too much will be overstimulating. ; (b) toys should not involve many ! different actions. ι 4. lessons should involve motor activity. (a) this is true of therapy with all young children, but especially so with brain-injured, since it reduces the distractibility. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society page in 5. many of the children need training in auditory recognition and amplification of sound should help in this respect. (a) they perceive words as a whole, and can not isolate parts. 6. mirror work may be helpful. (a) may involve too many distractions. 7. moto-kinesthetic cues have been found to be helpful. 8. probably no group therapy for a while. 9. if a child can read or write, these skills can be used in helping him speak. 10. many of the child's emotional needs can be met in speech therapy — make him feel secure and adequate. 11. always keep in mind the organismic approach and remember that anything that affects a part of the organism will affect the organism as a whole. 12. if a child is tending to perseverate, quietly shift to another, completely different activity, rather than attempting to continue that one. 13. music and rhythms may be of great advantage. 14. strauss and lehtinen feel that the use of situations such as a store, post office, or bank is not propitious with brain-injured children in academic education, since they involve many distractions and excitations with which he can not cope. this does not mean that learning by doing is not useful, but it need not be in terms of life situations; range of activity should be small. although there seems to be no literature on the subject, it is probable that situational frames of reference should not be employed in speech therapy. conclusions and implications since the etiology and diagnosis of minimal diffuse brain damage is notjclearcut and positive, the possibility of such a diagnosis might be considered when children with behavior problems, thinking and perceptual problems are presented to the speech clinic with this consideration therapeutic procedures will be quite different than they would be if there was not a possibility of brain damage. it is the author's belief that many children having slight brain injuries and classified as "ornery" or "spoiled" are not understood and may try to receive response by causing disturbances. if these children can be diagnosed and can be provided with a controlled environment and proper therapy, a part of our juvenile delinquency probfem may be solved. §§ b i b l i o g r a p h y 1. f r a z e u r , h . a . a n d h o a k l e y , p . , "significance of p s y c h o l o g i c a l ' test r e s u l t s of e x o g e n o u s a n d e n d o g e n o u s c h i l d r e n , " american journal of mental deficiency, 51:384-388, 1947. 2. g o l d s t e i n , k u r t , " c o n c e r n i n g r i g i d i t y , " character and personality 11:209-226, 1942-3. 3. j e l l i n e k , a u g u s t a , " p h e n o m e n a r e s e m b l i n g a p h a s i a , a g n o s i a , a n d a p r a x i a in m e n t a l l y defective c h i l d r e n a n d a d o l e s c e n t s , " journal o l speech disorders, 6.51-66. 1940. 4. n a n c e , l o r n a s . , "differential d i a g n o s i s of a p h a s i a in c h i l d r e n , " journal of speech disorders, 11:219-224, 1946. 4. s t r a u s s , alfred α . , a n d l e h t i n e n , l a u r a e . , psychopathology and education o l the brain-injured child, g r u n e & s t r a t t o n , n . y . , 1950. 6. s t r a u s s , a . a . and w e r n e r , h . , " d i s o r d e r s of c o n c e p t u a l t h i n k i n g in the b r a i n i n i u r e d c h i l d , " journal o l nervous mental disorders, 96:153, 1942. 7. w a l l i n , j . e., ch.ldren with mental and physical handicaps, p r e n t i c e h a l l , i n c . n . y . , 1949. 8. w e r n e r , h . , " t h e c o n c e p t of rigidity: a c r i t i c a l e v a l u a t i o n , " psychological review. 53:43, 1946. by r u t h m . c l a r k . p h . d . , a s s o c i a t e professor a n d d i r e c t o r , c h i l d r e n ' s speech clinic, university of d e n v e r , 2045 s o . y o r k s t r e e t . d e n v e r 19, c o l o r a d o . d r . c l a r k is a n h o n o r a r y m e m b e r of t h e s o u t h africa l o g o p e d i c s society, a n d lectured at t h e u n i v e r s i t y of w i t w a t e r s r a n d 1950-1951. §§ r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a p r i l j o u b n a l o p t h e s o u t h a ^ r i c a n l o g o p g b i c s o c i e t y <1 stuttering as learned behaviour theoretical and therapeutic implications by margaret marks, b.a., log. (rand), lt.c.l no theory concerning stuttering is justifiable without practical therapeutic implications. it is for this reason that i like to consider stuttering as a learned pattern of behaviour, the assumption being that what is learned can be unlearned, if not by the organism itself, then with the aid of therapeutic techniques. in attempting to discuss various authoritys' theories in learning terms i have divided theories into "exogenous" and "endogenous" groups—exogenous being these theories which postulate that the stuttering originated outside the organism; and endogenous being those which suggest that stuttering begin within the organism. it is self-explanatory that all the exogenous theorists (e.g., blumel (1); johnson (2) ; stein (3) fletcher (4)) would agree that something happens to the organism after birth which results in stuttering, i.e., that he leanis this stuttering pattern of behaviour. travis, 1931 (5), who is probably the most extreme of the endogenous group, sites unfavourable environmental factors as being contributory to the development of stuttering —"such accessory factors as prolonged emotional excitement, exhaustion and emotional shock may act upon the sub-soil of an insufficient cerebral dominance, and become the precipitating factors which allow the predisposing factors to manifest themselves in observable behaviour." he regards secondary symptoms as "largely reactions to, rather than an intergal part of, the stuttering condition." writers who believe in a precipitating condition which j acts upon a predisposing organism (e.g., karlin (6), in his "psycho-somatic theory; vari riper (7), in his predisposing— precipitating· and maintaining factors theory ; kingdon ward (8), in her theory of environment actings upon an organism with a "residual diathesis") all take into account the part played by the environment on the weak organism. these writers would also support the view that learning plays a large part in stuttering. if the actual stutterer can be divided into the original "block" or repetition," plus the "secondary symptom," we can consider that, while a few authorities consider the primary stage of stuttering to be a function of the organism, and not of its environment; all authorities seem to be of the opinion that the second stage of stuttering involves learning— the learning of (inadequate) patterns of behaviour which were originally intended to "avoid, postpone, disguise, start or release the speech abnormality." (van riper.) it can be argued, therefore, that the principles of learning can be applied to reduce these secondary symptoms of stuttering, if not to the actual elimination of the original "primary" stutter, which may or may not, be a function of learning. before discussing how this could be done, a brief outline of the fundamentals of learning is indicated. hull (9) (in his "principles of behaviour") gives as the four fundamentals of learning, drive, cue, response and reward. the drive—is a strong stimulus which impels the organism to action. it may be primary (physiological) or secondary (acquired). secondary drives are acquired on the basis of the primary drives, e.g., anxiety and fear are secondary drives based on the primary drive, pain. without drives the organism does not behave, and hence does not learn. cue : the drive compels a person to respond. cues determine when he will respond, where he will respond, and which response he will make. response: the way in which the person responds to a cue, when he is driven to a reward, is his response. reward: this is an event which produces a reduction in the drive like the drive it may be primary (physiological) or secondary (acquired) on the basis of a primary drive. without reward, there is no learning. to summarise the learning process — the drive impels a response which is determined by a cue. if this response is followed by a reward frequently enough, learning occurs, so that on the presentation of a certain situation (drive and cue) a particular response is made. i have attempted to rephrase elsewhere (10) some of the stuttering experts' theories in terms of these four fundamentals, e.g., johnson's theory: r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e f y a p r i l drive: to avoid non-fluency—"to avoid expected stuttering"—johnson. cue : "the expectation of stuttering"—johnson, 'words which cause anxiety about stuttering"—brown. response: "tension, an expression of the anxiety and an attempt to avoid the anxiety . . . an anticipatory hypertonic, avoidance reaction' '—johnson. reward : the uttering of the word. most rewards can be stated in fletcher's words as "communication, the basis of society." to get the word out is the primary aim, or drive, of the stutterer. the acquisition of secondary symptoms could be described in learning terms as follows: communication—to get his words out—is the stutterer's drive. he responds to the cue of a feared word or situation by a "hypertonic" speech effort—i.e., he forces the word out, often using bizarre accompanying movements. eventually he is rewarded,, i.e., he gets the word out. then there comes into play the "gradient in the effect of reward"— i.e., if a number of different responses are made to a cue, and that the last of these responses is followed by reward, the connection to the last response will be strengthened most. therefore, a movement, a gesture, a vocalisation, which may have nothing to do with the actual utterance of the word, may be "learned" as it was the nearest action in time to the reward response. the act of stuttering may be specifically reinforced by virtue of its relatively close association with anxiety tension reduction accompanying the removal of a feared word. it seems as if there is a vicious circle where completion of the stuttered act results in a reduction of the anxiety tension evoked by the stimulus word, with consequent reinforcement of stuttering behaviour. secondary symptoms appear to be learned by the stutterer to help him get over a block. especially when first acquired, secondary symptoms probably serve largely as anxietyreducing agents. if we wish to change the stutterer's response, we must attempt to change either his drives, his cues, or his rewards (or all of these). established therapies have been directed towards these goals. mental hygiene and re-evaluation have aimed at changing the stutterer's drives and cues. negative practice of secondary symptoms aims at substituting a punishment for a reward when the response of the secondary symptom is made. i feel that stuttering therapy could be handled with greater confidence if we worked systematically on the basis of learning. the stutterer can be helped to react differently to cues (particularly feared situations) if he is helped to make a better general adjustment (e.g., encouraging "excitatory" traits in "inhibitory" personalities, salter (11); he could learn to respond to situations with relaxation instead of tension (jacobson's progressive relaxation (12) ; wolpe's reciprocal inhibition and specific desensitisation (12). as in negative practice, his secondary symptoms could acquire a punishment value, rather than a reward value (e.g., as the reward of a secondary stutterer is the utterance of the word, i am attempting to make the stutterer respond to a secondary symptom with a complete silence—the secondary " —" acquires punishment value by breaking communication, instead of reward value by continuing it). once therapists have accepted the principles of learning psychology, the possibilities of systematic therapy are limitless. as kingdon ward says "if we accept the factor of heredity we are committed to a passive acceptance of conditions which can never be wholly overcome—the post-natal environment can be modified to some extent." i believe that a scientific manipulation of the environment (drives, cues and rewards) and responses of a learning organism; can be one of our best leads in stuttering therapy. 1. bluemel, c.s.—"stammering and allied disorders," 1935. 2. johnson, w.—"speech handicapped school children," 1948. 3. stein, l.—"speech and voice," 1942. 4. fletcher, j. m.—"the problem of stuttering," 1928, 5. travis—"speech pathology," 1931. 6. karlin : psychosomatic theory of stuttering— "journal of speech disorders," 1947. 7. van riper—"speech correction," 1954. 8. kindon ward, w.—"stammering—a contribution to the study of its problems and treatment," 1941. 9. hull, c. c.—"principles of behaviour," 1954. 10. marks, m.—"stuttering behaviour and learning". thesis presented for b.a. log. degree, 1953. 11. salter—"conditioned reflex therapy." 12. jacobson—"progressive relaxation." 13. wolpe—reciprocal inhibition as the main basis of theraputic elfects : "arch. neurol, psychiat." 1954. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the identification process in early communication intervention (eci) by primary health care personnel in ditsobotla sub—district. jeannie van der linde, alta kritzinger, aniei redelinghuys university of pretoria abstract based on clinical observations, case finding for early communication intervention service delivery in rural areas, such ditsobotla sub-district, is limited. the study described the identification process used for infants and young children at risk for communication delay and disorders as part of a proposed incremental implementation of early communication inter vention services within the existing primary health care package. a descriptive survey design was followed. a rating scale was utilized and interviews were conducted with 20 randomly selected primary health care personnel and eight primary health care programme managers in ditsobotla sub-district in north west province. the aims were to describe the early identification methods/processes, resources and limitations and provide guidelines to introduce early communication in tervention services in a rural community. the results indicated that the identification process was limited. an incremental implementation of the different early communication intervention functions within the primary health care package ap­ pears feasible. implications of the findings may be applied to initiate early communication intervention services, based on integrated teamwork, in a rural district in south africa. key words: early communication intervention, early identification, referral systems, case finding, infants and young chil­ dren, at-risk, communication delay or disorders, primary health care r ural communities in south africa particularly bear a high burden of disability, which may be both the cause and the consequence of poverty (emmett, 2005). infants at risk of developmental delay and their families who live in poverty therefore face additional constraints that place them at increased risk for developmental disabilities (moodley, 1999). due to a worldwide increase in developmental disabilities in chil­ dren under three years (rossetti, 2001), it can be expected that the number of young children presenting with communication delay and disorders will also increase. multiple risks of communication delay and disorders in young children occur in the rural sub-district of ditsobotla in the north west province. from january to march 2005, 511 infants were born in the local hospitals and approximately 18% of them weighed less than 2500g (monthly hospital statistics, north west province, 2005), which indicates low birth weight and a concomi­ tant risk of developmental delay and disability (rossetti, 2001). according to the same hospital statistics, 5% of the mothers were adolescents, which poses a risk for maladaptive parenting and poverty if they are not supported by other women (werner, 2000) and 2.5% of the mothers had syphilis, a risk of bilateral sen­ sorineural hearing loss of sudden onset and progression in their infants (plante & beeson, 2004). approximately 5% of the infants were born to mothers with hiv/aids (monthly hospital statistics, north west province, 2005), with a risk of mother-to-child trans­ mission of the virus, and further risks of health and developmen­ tal difficulties, congenital hearing loss or the development of hearing impairment shortly after birth (swanepoel, 2004). de­ spite the different risk conditions and the clear indication to establish a sustainable early communication intervention (eci) programme in the sub-district, very few infants at risk were identified and referred to the first author during a year of com­ munity service employment. based on the one example of eci services in south africa not sufficiently meeting the needs of the mother and infant risk population, and a call from various authors to expand service delivery to include community-based eci services (kritzinger & louw, 2003; moodley, 1999; pickering et al., 1998), a research project was launched (van der linde, 2008). the aim was to investigate the existing identification and referral practices used in primary health care (phc) services of the specific sub­ district in order to determine needs and resources that should be considered when developing eci services. contact: prof. alta kritzinger department o f communication pathology university o f pretoria pretoria e-mail: alta.kritzinger@up.ac.za 48 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) mailto:alta.kritzinger@up.ac.za the id e n t if ic a t io n p r o c e s s in the underpinnings for the research study . „ mpreed over time in the field of early intervention. hence that ei iic s oariv intervention was shown to be linked to the the efficacy or eariy . „ at identification (rossetti, 2001). if under three years child s age the window of opportunity may be enhanced by neurode ° lopmental plasticity (dennis, 2000). early identification of risk conditions in infants should therefore be the primary function of i (kritzinger, louw & rossetti, 2001). developing and imple­ menting effective identification strategies, especially in rural ar­ eas is one of the biggest challenges in eci, as a lack of identifica­ tion will compromise the efficiency of the services (kritzinger, 2000). consequently, the efficacy of early identification of an infant at risk of developmental delay depends on a reliable refer­ ral system so that time delays between the detection of a prob­ lem and intervention services to the family may be minimised. furtherm ore, th e efficacy of a referral system is in direct rela­ tion to the outcomes that the system a ch ie ve s (equity project, 2000). by means of a smoothly functioning referral system the eci client and family, for example, receives diagnostic and th e ra ­ peutic services from a specialised team of medical and re h a b ilita ­ tive professionals, ensuring th a t phc is more unified and finan­ cially sustainable (equity project, 2000; n orth west department of health, 2003). it appears that limited referrals and improper coordination of referral systems are partially responsible for eci and its benefits being unknown in the south african health care system (kritzinger, 2000). integrating eci services into phc in rural communities in south africa has been recommended in various studies (fair & louw, 1999; kritzinger & louw, 2003; moodley, 1999). formalised by rossetti in 1996, the concept of communication-based services to the population of children birth to three years of age, is based on the finding that communication delay is the most prevalent characteristic of delayed development in children in this age group (rossetti, 2001). enhancing communication development should therefore be the main focus of any effective early interven­ tion programme (rossetti, 2001) hence the term early commu­ nication intervention. in the planning and management of eci services, the focus should be to develop strategies to prevent communication disor­ ders in all people, including rural and disadvantaged communi­ ties (kritzinger, 2000). as prevention of disability and communi­ cation disorders is the ultimate goal in eci, primary prevention programmes could be implemented at phc clinics. a transdisciplinary framework for eci public service delivery has been proposed by kritzinger and louw (2003), which may be integrated into the phc package of the department of health (2000). the phc package was designed to provide comprehen­ sive and integrated health services and not to be implemented through separate, vertical programmes where services are split primary health care personnel in ditsobotla sub-district according to disciplines, resulting in barriers to teamwork (van rensburg, 2004). consisting of eight different programmes, the package entails a standardised and comprehensive ‘basket’ of preventative, promotive, basic curative, rehabilitative and pallia­ tive services delivered at district level (department of health, 2000). linked together as a service delivery model for health care, there are eight programmes i.e., 1) non-personal health services (occupational and environmental health); 2) disease prevention and control; 3) maternal, child and women's health; 4) hiv/aids, sexually transmitted infections and tuberculosis; 5) health monitoring and evaluation; 6) mental health and sub­ stance abuse; 7) gender issues, and 8) school health services. these should be mutually supported by the health care profes­ sionals delivering these services (department of health, 2000; van rensburg, 2004). since teamwork is an essential approach in the implementation of the phc package, the opportunity for collaboration should be utilised by therapists to introduce eci services in communities. the transdisciplinary conceptual framework for eci describes collaborative partnerships with parents and caregivers, commu­ nity health nurses and doctors, speech-language therapists, audi­ ologists at phc clinics, and secondary caregivers at creches and day-care facilities (kritzinger & louw, 2003). according to kritz­ inger and louw (2003) eci service delivery may be introduced to be consistent with the developmental stages of young children and the contexts where caregivers can be reached. examples include services to the mother at antenatal clinics before the infant is born (providing preventative information on normal hearing and communication development); during the neonatal period in the postnatal ward (promoting mother-infant, attach­ ment and interaction, hearing screening and identifying risks for communication delay); during the postnatal period at immuniza­ tion clinics (follow-up hearing screening, identifying further risks and provide eci, advocacy for education); and during the toddler years at day-care centres and nursery schools (providing eci, training parents, facilitating a language and emergent literacy rich preschool curriculum to facilitate school readiness) (kritzinger & louw, 2003). with the exception of non-personal health services, integrating direct eci services into seven of the different phc programmes appears to be possible. the wide-ranging eci functions such as promotion of normal development, prevention of feeding difficul­ ties, hearing loss and communication disorders, parent training, screening, early identification and providing assessment and intervention correspond with the different services provided within the variety of programmes in the phc package. if certain eci functions are shared among professionals in phc, case finding of infants at risk for communication delays or disorders may be improved and appropriate eci services may be rendered to families at phc facilities. examples of successful integration of early communication intervention followed by were based on evithe south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 49 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) jeannie van der linde, alta kritzinger, aniel redelinghuys paediatric health and developmental services in phc can be found (lequerica, 1997). according to lequerica (1997) it ap­ pears that such a one-stop phc service delivery model is pre­ ferred by families, and increases family participation. it is there­ fore important to investigate the feasibility of the integration of phc and eci programmes in a south african context where eci services are lacking. since the primary goal of research is to improve understanding of a phenomenon (leedy & ormrod, 2005), information produced by the current research study aims to identify certain problem areas. the research may also provide guidelines for identification and referral practices for eci in the phc context, which may be used to improve case finding in rural areas in south africa. the information gathered through the research may also support the facilitation of a transdisciplinary team approach to service deliv­ ery in eci in ditsobotla sub-district, where effective collaboration between the phc nurses, speech-language therapists, audiolo gists, and phc programme managers may be established. method aims and objectives the purpose of the study was to describe the characteristics of identification methods and referral practices for eci currently followed by phc personnel in the ditsobotla sub-district. the following objectives were formulated: □ to describe the features of the facilities in the phc con­ text in order to determine the capacity for implementing an eci programme for infants with risks for disabilities. g to describe the human resources available in order to determine the needs within the phc context. g to describe the early identification methods and re­ sources currently used by the phc personnel and man­ agers to detect health problems as well as developmen­ tal delay and disabilities in infants. [] to describe the referral systems, between different organizational departments in one facility, and the net­ works between different institutions, which are currently being used by phc personnel for eci services. g to describe the phc programme managers' views on how to implement an early identification and referral programme for infants with risks for disabilities and delays in an integrated phc and eci approach. research design a descriptive research study was conducted. a dominant-less dominant research design was selected, which entailed the use of a quantitative approach, the dominant component, and a qualitative approach, the smaller or less-dominant component (fouche & de vos, 2005). the quantitative approach implied a formalized approach with explicit control during data-collection (fouche & de vos, 2005). a questionnaire and a rating scale were used as methods of data collection. the qualitative compo­ nent consisted of semi-structured interviews with programme managers to obtain an in-depth understanding of data already collected and that may have needed further clarification (leedy & ormrod, 2005). to increase the reliability of the study, triangulation was used (de vos, 2005). triangulation results from a combination of qualitative and quantitative research approaches. it is based on the assumption that bias in a particular data source, investigator or method may be neutralized when used in combination with other data sources, investigators or methods (de vos, 2005). in the current study both data triangulation, i.e. more than one data source in this case the phc sisters and the phc programme managersand methodological triangulation, i.e. multiple meth­ ods or approaches such as a rating scale and face-to-face inter­ views, were used (de vos, 2005). research ethics the research proposal was approved by the research ethics and proposal committee of the faculty of humanities, university of pretoria. permission to conduct the research was granted by the north west department of health. all participants gave voluntary informed consent to participate in the study. information obtained from the participants was treated confidentially and reported anonymously. all efforts were made to conduct the research according to the ethical principles of no harm to participants, veracity, non-discrimination and sensi­ tivity to cultural and language differences between participants and the researcher. , participants the two target populations were phc personnel and phc pro­ gramme managers who were employed by ditsobotla sub-district, i which is part of the central district of north west department of i health. both groups had to be proficient in english in or^er to participate in the study. although setswana is the most com­ monly used language in the sub-district, it was found that the participants were sufficiently proficient in english to understand and answer the interview questions. selection of participants stratified random sampling was used to select the participants in group 1, the phc personnel (strydom.& venter, 2002). stratifiy cations entailed that the specific population was divided in strata, which were homogeneous with regard to some characteristics (strydom & venter, 2002). the entire phc personnel of ditsobotla sub-district were divided into two strata according to the size of the phc facilities: all the clinics operating for 8 hours and 10 hours per day were part of stratum 1, while all the phc hospitals 50 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the identification process in early communication intervention followed by primary health care personnel in ditsobotla sub-district and 24-hour clinics were part of stratum 2. the selection within the strata was random (strydom & venter, 2002) as the re­ searcher randomly selected the facilities to be included in each stratum. in group 2 all the phc programme managers of ditsobotla sub­ district who complied with the selection criteria received informa­ tion brochures and were invited to participate. in this case the number of possible participants was limited. non-random pur­ posive sampling was used since a group of participants to repre­ sent the population did not have to be identified (leedy & orm rod, 2005). description of participants -description of participants in group 1: approximately one third (20) of the phc personnel in the entire sub-district were included in the study, with each stratum equally represented. the charac­ teristics of the participants in group 1 are summarised in table 1. according to table 1 there were only a few differences between the participants of the two strata. the majority of the participants had nursing diplomas, all were working on a full-time basis, with an average of eight to 12 years working experience. the partici­ pants therefore had work experience which rendered them as informed research participants, although three participants had no formal training. table 1. characteristics of the participants in group 1 (n=20) course. the characteristics of the participants are summarised in table 2. according to table 2 most of the programme managers had qualifications and work experience. a variety of programmes in the phc package were represented by the managers, but those who managed the maternal, child and women’s health, mental health and the hiv/aids programmes could not be included as participants as they were attending a course during the time allo­ cated for data collection. table 2. characteristics of the participants in group 2 (n=8) c h a r a cter istic s tr a ta 1 (n= 10) s tr a ta 2 (n= 10) q u alificatio n 2: b c u r d eg ree 6: d ip lo m a 2: n o fo rm al tra in ­ ing i 3: b c u r d egree 6: d iplom a 1: n o fo rm al tra in ­ ing y ears o f ex p e ri­ ence 4 y ears 25 years a v erag e: 12,5 years i 1 y ears 26 years a v erag e: 8,4 years f irs t language 10: s etsw an a i 9: s etsw an a 1: a frik a an s e m p lo y m en t status 10: f u ll-tim e 10: f u ll-tim e t ype o f fac ility w h ere em p lo y ed 7: 8 -h o u r clin ic 3: 12-hour clinic 2: p h c h o sp ital 8: 2 4 -h o u r clinic qualifications masters de­ gree, b cur degree, nursing diploma to no formal training range: 1 lo y average: 4y experi­ ence in pro­ gramme managing phc facili­ ties visited hospitals clinics frail care centres prisons hospices schools creches business premises first lan­ guage 2: afri­ kaans 6 : setswa na all full­ time em­ ploy­ ment status phc pro­ gramme man­ aged geriatrics, chronic dis­ eases rehabilitation nutrition communicable diseases occupational therapy health promo­ tion nutrition school health environmental health description of participants in group 2 the participants in group 2 represented more than two thirds of the total of the phc programme managers. a total of 12 phc programme managers were working in ditsobotla sub-district. all the programme managers were asked to participate, and a pilot study was conducted with one of the programme managers prior to the data collection. that left 11 phc programme managers, eight of whom participated in the research. the other three pomaterial and apparatus the following material was exclusively developed for the data collection of the study: a summated rating scale for the description of the facilities (see appendix a). summated rating scales are widely used for the description of environments where phenomena have to be evaluated on a con­ tinuum of, for example, excellent to inadequate (leedy & ormrod, 2005). the rating scale used was a means to analyse facilities and describe the available human resources. the rating scale provided not only the background to the study, but also relevant data which could point to strengths and weaknesses in the identi­ fication methods and referral systems of the phc system in the ditsobotla sub-district. the rating scale was also used to deter­ mine the capacity of the facilities to implement the different eci functions. the content of the rating scale included the following physical characteristics: water and electricity supply, the educational at­ mosphere of the waiting rooms, developmental screening materi­ als available, space for interviews and screening assessments, filing cabinets and filing system. tential participants were either on leave or were attending a the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 51 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the human resources and services to infants were also re­ corded for each study site. face-to-face interviews to investigate the identification processes used by the participants in group 1 a structured interview was used to collect data on the identifi­ cation processes, which included questions on the identification methods and the referral systems used by phc personnel to iden­ tify and refer infants with developmental disorders or who are at risk for developmental delay. when the focus of a study is to in­ vestigate a process, in this case the identification process, struc­ tured interviews are especially suitable (greeff, 2002). although participants may not be as truthful in their responses as in the case of self-administered questionnaires, face-to-face interviews have the advantage of the highest response rate in survey re­ search (leedy & ormrod, 2005). face-to-face interviews with the participants in group 2 on the implementation of an eci programme with its emphasis on early identification a set of individual interviews with the participants in group 2 was necessary in order to triangulate data, which brought differ­ ent views on the current identification methods, referral systems and teamwork to the fore and therefore led to more accurate descriptions of the same aspects. furthermore the phc pro­ gramme managers’ opinion on the implementation of the differ­ ent eci functions within the phc package was obtained. the apparatus used during all the interviews w as an o lym p u s v n 240 p c digital voice recorder. t h e digital voice files were tran­ scribed and securely stored on a laptop computer. the first author w as the only person to have access to the data. procedures reliability a nd validity with triangulation the reliability of the data was increased, as it was collected by means of different instruments and results could therefore be compared. furthermore, two separate groups of participants were used to investigate the identification meth­ ods and referral systems, while the researcher’s own field notes further supported the data obtained in the interviews.' a second reviewer was utilized to verify the interview recordings of both groups of participants. the reviewer listened to the re­ cordings while reading the transcriptions. discrepancies between the text and voice recordings were discussed until agreement was found between the researcher and second reviewer. face validity of the instruments was ensured as the format of the measuring instruments corresponded with the objectives of the research. all questions of the interview schedules were rele­ vant and clearly formulated to ensure content validity. a senior speech-language therapist with working experience in primary health care reviewed the questionnaire and made suggestions to improve the relevancy of the questions. pilot studies o f the rating scale and interview schedules jeannie van der linde, alta kritzinger, aniel redelinghuys pilot studies were conducted to test the rating scale and the two interview schedules in order to increase the accuracy and reliability of the data. according to the selection criteria, two phc nurses and one phc programme manager were selected to be part of the pilot studies. the rating scale and interview schedules were adapted according to the results obtained in the pilot stud­ ies. procedures o f the m ain stu d y based on the two strata, the different facilities to be included in the study were randomly selected from a total of 17 phc sites in the sub-district. ditsobotla sub-district, previously named lichten burg is part of the greater taung local municipality and forms the central district in the north west province. the district is largely rural and has an unemployment rate of over 50%. the main in­ dustries are mining and quarrying, but agriculture is the focus (escom, 2007). during the first visit to a facility, permission was obtained to complete the rating scale. the researcher then filled in the rating scale without being intrusive. the completed rating scale was verified by one of the participants at each site so that a true re­ port of the facility was stored for data analysis. after explaining the reason for the visit, the facility manager introduced the re­ searcher to the potential participants in group 1. informed con­ sent was obtained from the participants in group 1 and the struc­ tured interviews were conducted according to the interview schedules. the interviews were conducted at the participants’ working facilities, in order to ensure that the participants did not have to travel and that they would be as comfortable as possible. the interviews were conducted in a consultation room at the fa­ cilities thereafter the researcher visited the participants of group 2 at the district office of ditsobotla sub-district. informed consent was obtained and the semi-structured interviews were conducted in each participant’s office. this made the participants feel comfort­ able and little hindrance was present due to the professional environment in which the interviews were conducted. data analysis \ the data of the structured interviews were analysed first, in order to determine which aspects needed further investigation and had to be included in the semi-structured interview schedule with participants of group 2. the data obtained from the first interviews were analysed quantitatively according to the sas system version 9.1 statistical computer software. as the analysis was done in microsoft excel, the data were stored electronically (babbie, 2004). ^ frequencies were calculated as data should be presented in a condensed form when the data consisted of a large number of observations (steyn, smit, du toit, & strasheim, 1998). basic graphical presentations were used to better understand the data 52 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the identification process in early communication intervention followed by primary health care personnel in ditsobotla sub-district posters or brochures on child development. only five clinics had television monitors for educational videos. and decide where further statistical analysis would be appropri­ ate. fisher’s exact test of independence was used for data with two attribute variables (leedy & ormrod, 2005). the rating scales were analysed numerically, as the data collec­ tion method was quantitative. procedures similar to those used to analyse the data from the structured interviews were therefore used for the analysis of the rating scale. in qualitative research the data analysis and interpretation are closely connected (leedy & ormrod, 2005). this had to be taken into account when the results of the semi-structured interviews were analysed and interpreted. the researcher listened to the voice recordings and supplemented the transcriptions with field notes. data were holistically read and re-read before the corpus was categorised according to themes. according to leedy and ormrod (2005) the researcher has to have a heightened and focused awareness of the data and subtle undercurrents during interviews have to be identified. regularities were noted during the data analysis and categories of meaning emerged that could be presented as descriptive results. results and discussion the results are presented and discussed in accordance with the objectives of the study. the capacity of the facilities for implementing an early identifi­ cation programme for infants at risk of disabilities the basic features of the facility, the waiting rooms and consult­ ing rooms were evaluated according to the rating scale (see ap­ pendix a) and described to determine the possibility of imple­ menting eci functions in these facilities. primary prevention ac­ tivities, such as talks to caregivers to promote normal communi­ cation and emergent literacy development, can possibly be imple­ mented in the waiting rooijns of the facilities, while identification and intervention of infants and young children, i.e. primary and secondary prevention, may be implemented in the consultation rooms. j the checklist results indicated that all phc facilities which were investigated had electricity and water supply, although some water shortages and power breaks occur. all buildings were per­ manent structures and three clinics were recently renovated. the travelling distance from lichtenburg, the town in the centre of the sub-district, is not more than 35 km. most of the roads are not tarred and the public transport is by minibus taxis. the implica­ tion is that a speech-language therapist or an audiologist may be able to visit two clinics in close approximation on one day. the results also indicated that not all facilities have the capac­ ity to implement eci functions in waiting rooms and/or consulta­ tion rooms, but patient information was adequately protected by means of secured filing cabinets. six of the ten clinics had limited seating space in the waiting rooms, which means that educa­ tional talks may not be possible. none of the waiting rooms had as only a few of the facilities have the capacity for all eci func­ tions to be implemented, an incremental implementation of eci services could be introduced. speech-language therapists and audiologists therefore need to determine which levels of preven­ tion can be implemented at which facilities in the sub-district. it may be possible that one facility only has the capacity to support the implementation of the primary prevention level in eci ser­ vices, while all three levels of prevention can be implemented at another facility. since the waiting room capacity in the majority of the clinics was limited, it may imply that educational talks cannot be given and that only individual screening, assessment and in­ tervention can be offered at the clinic. promotion of communica­ tion development and other information may be given to caregiv­ ers in the form of brochures in the local language. the availability, needs and limitations of human resources with-in the phc context a shortage of phc personnel will have an impact on the incre­ mental implementation of eci functions. since the phc personnel may not be able to assist the speech-language therapist and audiologist in some of the eci functions, the capacity and support of the human resources at the facilities need to be determined in order to establish which eci functions can be effectively imple­ mented in the facilities. according to the results there were community health nurses present at all the phc facilities investigated, but not all were qualified at the time of the investigation. there were limited health care professionals such as doctors, speech-language therapists, audiologists and paediatricians working in the sub­ district. although an inadequate arrangement, an itinerant doctor visited nine different phc clinics at least once a week. the tenth phc facility is a hospital where a permanent doctor was em­ ployed at the time of data collection. the phc personnel had limited knowledge regarding the avail­ ability of allied health care professionals at their facilities. the lack of knowledge of the different health care professionals work­ ing in the district may reflect an inadequate teamwork approach. ineffective communication and collaboration between phc nurses and other health care professionals, limited information regarding available services and no in-service training regarding the scope of practice of specialized services were further indica­ tors that an inadequate teamwork approach is present in ditsobotla sub-district. it was estimated by the participants that, on average, approxi­ mately 165 infants were seen at the ten different phc facilities on a daily basis. it is clear that the infant population of the sub­ district may be accessed through the phc facilities. limited hu­ man resources appear to be one of the reasons why screening for the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 53 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) jeannie van der linde, alta kritzinger, aniel redelinghuys communication and hearing disorders was not carried out in ditsobotla sub-district. the participants felt that it was the doc­ tors’ responsibility to screen for hearing and communication dis­ orders in young children as they are trusted by caregivers. an interesting finding was that the participants felt that parents and family members share responsibility in identifying communication delays or disorders in the child's development. human resources and an adequate team approach appear to be limited in ditsobotla sub-district. by implementing eci activi­ ties on a primary prevention level at all phc facilities the following may be achieved: introduction of speech-language therapy and audiology, promotion of eci services to both the phc profession­ als and the caregivers and provision of information on early iden­ tification, stimulation of infants and young children and communi­ cation developmental milestones. the introduction of eci to the phc professionals and caregivers may provide the basis on which the incremental implementation of other eci activities, such as developmental surveillance, can be achieved at phc facilities that have the capacity to accommodate such eci activities. eci implementation may be established by coordinating the services with certain phc programmes, i.e. non-personal health services, disease prevention and control, maternal, child and women’s health, hiv/aids, sexually transmitted infections and tuberculo­ sis, mental health and substance abuse, and health monitoring and evaluation. the early identification methods and resources currently used by the phc personnel posters on general health issues were numerous in the facili­ ties, but posters on infant and child development were absent. there is a great need to develop mass communication material in setswana and english for eci in this sub-district which has to be addressed by speech-language therapists and audiologists. the results clearly indicated that none of the facilities had checklists to use for developmental surveillance and therefore it appeared that phc professionals were not conducting any formal develop­ mental screening of infants and young children. appropriate checklists and case history forms for different south african con­ texts need to be developed so that children at risk of develop­ mental delays can be successfully detected and referred within the phc facilities (department of health, 2000). the results indicated that phc personnel use observation and that they talk to the child to determine if there is a problem in his/her communication development. talking to the child to de­ termine the level of communication development is a positive initiative, but the participants indicated that they do not have a guideline to assist them in identifying atypical development. the informal method of talking to the child which is currently used in the sub-district does not appear to be valid and the phc person­ nel are in need of locally developed, valid, reliable and easy to use identification methods. the participants clearly indicated a need for formalized and valid identification methods and in service training on how to identify infants and young children with communication delay or hearing impairment. the participants viewed their identification method as largely ineffective and unre­ liable. baby clinics, that form part of the mother, child and women’s health programme, were identified by the majority of the phc personnel as the best phc programme to implement the devel­ opmental surveillance of communication abilities in infants. the referral systems used by phc personnel to refer patients to speech-language therapists and audiologists. according to figure 1 the referral route is clear, but some diffi­ culties in making referrals are experienced. a total of 80% of the participants indicated that they experienced time delays when patients had been referred. a disturbing finding was that caregiv­ ers of infants and young children were asked to come back a number of times before the referral to the speech-language therapist was made. the distances between patients’ homes and the hospitals need to be taken into consideration, as patients usually cannot afford to travel far on a regular basis. due to the figure 1. the referral process used by participants to refer patients for speech-language therapy and audiology services i large number of patients to be seen by speech-language thera­ pists and audiologists and the transport problems, scheduled follow-up visits are irregular in the sub-district. therefore it would be best for the patient to receive eci services at the local clinic, instead of at the secondary health care hospjtal which may require further, more costly travel. participants in both group 1 and 2 felt that the referral process was not effective and their reasons were as follows: []-a ck of communication between professionals 54 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) intervention followed by primary health care personnel in ditsobotla sub-district teamwork: 03etter communication and cooperation between professionals [ j h e referral information is limited, resulting in the profes­ sional asking for the case history from the clinic [ j e w professionals, such as speech-language therapists, are currently working in the sub-district; thus only limited services are available in the district, and phc personnel are uncertain of whether services are available [referral books are not available for eci, and the phc person­ nel do not get feedback after the referral has been made [3-ong distances to the hospitals □the patients struggle with transport as they have little or no money for their trip [ j h e phc personnel receive little or no feedback from the speech-language therapists on their referrals the doctors visit clinics only a few times every week, which delays the referral process as the patient is referred to the doctor for an appointment the results from the current study indicated that few referrals were made to speech-language therapy and audiology services, which differs from developed contexts, such as the uk (enderby & petheram, 2000). in the uk many patients are referred to local speech-language therapy and audiology services, but the referral process also faces certain challenges. factors influencing the referral process in the uk include limited knowledge of the risk factors for communication delay, long waiting periods for appoint­ ments, epidemiological and demographic factors, such as long travelling distances (enderby & petheram, 2000). the lack of human resources, financial constraints on families who need eci i services and limited teamwork as reasons for poor referrals may be specific to rural communities in developing contexts. the sug­ gestions for improvement of the referral process clearly indicate that the participants were concerned about the difficulties and are positive about the implementation of an effective referral system: j | administrative suggestion's: , qmproving back-referral system by implementing referral let­ ters where back-referral is mandatory q)evising a simple and easy-to-use referral form on which pa­ tient information can be written with space provided for back referral to the referring sister (in addition to the road to health chart). [employm ent of a full-time speech-language therapist in the sub-district provide feedback to the sub-district on the referral process and schedule regular meetingsiwith provincial and national coordina­ tor in order to develop a new referral process the identification process in early communication [^developing a team to address the needs of the infants and young children and their families in the community information and training: qmproving the awareness of the phc personnel and training them in how to identify and refer infants and young children at risk for communication delay, instead of waiting for the doctor to make the referrals qnform the public regarding the services and initiate work­ shops for parents and teachers the suggestions for improvement are useful to establish eci services in the sub-district. practical aspects as well as manage­ rial and planning issues were included in the recommendations. it is clear that it was valuable to include the programme manag­ ers as participants in the research. the current teamwork approach and participant views on the implementation of an early identification and referral programme, and the integration of eci and the phc programmes through col­ laborative activities all the phc personnel and phc programme managers indicated that collaboration with other professionals would have a positive influence on case finding in eci. the phc personnel indicated that collaboration would positively influence the identification and referral of infants and young children at risk for communication delay, as infants and young children can be identified earlier and time-delays can be decreased in the referral process. the inclu­ sion of volunteers in collaboration with the phc personnel in or­ der to improve identification of infants at risk of communication delays needs to be explored further as volunteers can make regu­ lar home visits in the community to support families with special needs. the majority of the phc nurses indicated that teamwork should occur during early identification, referral and the early interven­ tion phases of eci service delivery. consequently it is essential to establish commitment, competencies and a supportive environ­ ment to ensure an effective collaborative teamwork model, so as to positively reinforce the implementation of eci services in ditsobotla sub-district. the phc programme managers did not only provide relevant reasons for including the specific eci function but also indicated the areas where collaboration should be implemented. the re­ sults indicated that the phc programme managers have a clear understanding o f eci and of the importance of these services. their knowledge of eci may therefore positively influence the implementation of eci functions by means of collaborative activithe south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 55 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) jeannie van der linde, alta kritzinger, aniel redelinghuys ties in the district. since they understand the importance of early identification and eci, the phc programme managers appeared to be motivated and ready to assist in the implementation of such activities. consequently it is important to build on the partici­ pants’ interest in eci, which were stimulated by the current re­ search project. the programme managers discussed how and why the different eci functions should be implemented in ditsobotla sub-district: -to train m others to stimulate their infants and children the phc programme managers indicated that this function will be beneficial to the infants as well as the mothers. to screen the communication and hearing abilities in you n g children the majority of the phc programme managers indicated that this function can be integrated in the programmes. the school health programme was specified as a programme in which screening campaigns should be launched in order to identify young children in the following contexts: preschool playgroups, creches and in primary school. the health promotion programme does not need to be included in the screening of infants and young children. this function needs to be implemented at facility level and the speech-language therapist should work in collabora­ tion with the facility managers. -to determine risks for communication delay in you n g children and to provide intervention the phc programme managers indicated that this is an important function to be integrated as communication delays or disorders in infants and young children can be prevented or decreased. the programmes should be in­ cluded in the implementation of eci in the rural community as their resources are invaluable. infants and young children in need of special services have to be referred as soon as possible. the mother, child and women’s and the school health programmes are implemented in contexts such as pre-schools where it is fea­ sible to monitor the communication development of infants and young children. -to advocate the education o f children the phc programme managers indicated that communities should be made aware of the importance of education. this eci function may improve the future of many children. the school health programme needs to operate in collaboration with the schools in order to promote the importance of education. all programmes should be involved in promotional initiatives and the departments of health and educa­ tion need to collaborate. -to facilitate the im plem entation o f a language and literacy based preschool curriculum to ensure school readiness all the phc programme managers supported the integration of this func­ tion into the phc programmes. the departments of health and education need to collaborate on school readiness. the educa­ tors should be trained to stimulate the communication develop­ ment of young children and facilitate the skills to be acquired at preschool level. early learning centres and creches should be visited in collaboration with the school health programme. the training of the preschool personnel may also assist in identifying infants and young children at risk for communication delays. -to evaluate and m onitor the collaborative activities the phc programme managers explained that it is very important to dem­ onstrate progress and to identify the problem areas which may exist. statistics need to be monitored and the goals of the activi­ ties have to be kept in mind. the effectiveness of teamwork has to be evaluated and the phc programme managers have to have regular meetings in order to monitor progress in a collaborative way. -community awareness cam paigns on eci services and partici­ pation in health calendar day activities the eci programme should be integrated in the community by collaboration with the other phc programme initiatives. the importance of community campaigns as a way to provide focused information and to intro­ duce new programmes to people was emphasized by the phc programme managers. conclusion and implications since case finding and referrals are inadequate and eci ser­ vices have not yet been established in ditsobotla sub-district, certain limitations can be expected. the study found that the identification methods for infants at risk of communication delay or disorders are limited and unreliable, and that the referral sys­ tem appears to be ineffective in this impoverished rural district. against the background of difficulties to establish eci services in south africa, identified by kritzinger (2000), it appears that iden­ tification and referral are but two aspects of many influencing eci service delivery. inadequate access to eci facilities, the scarcity of facilities, limited knowledge about the benefits of eci among caregivers and health care professionals, and limited, employ­ ment of early communication interventionists (kritzinger, 2000) are other aspects which impact negatively on the professional obligation to expand eci services in south africa. 1 i in order to improve service delivery in rural areas and therefore make services more accessible to the communities, the findings indicated that the integration of eci functions within the phc package is feasible. the results of the study found an interest in eci among primary health care personnel and managers and a positive attitude to improve services. a formal approach to imple­ mentation was suggested, and collaborative partnerships be­ tween management, phc programme managers^and phc person­ nel are feasible in the sub-district. the incremental rollout of eci activities at phc facilities may also improve teamwork, as col­ laboration is essential to improve the efficacy of early identifica­ tion and referrals. kritzinger (2000) also described limited teamwork and limited 56 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the identification process in early communication intervention followed by primary health care personnel in ditsobotla sub-district knowledge of eci as problem areas in service delivery. the imple­ mentation of collaborative activities necessitates a commitment to integrated teamwork by all involved. the results of the current study showed a remarkable motivation by the participants for the implementation of eci. training of phc personnel in eci services and promotion of normal communication and literacy develop­ ment among young children form part of the different eci primary prevention functions. these functions may be phased in at phc facilities with sufficient capacity and human resources. further­ more, training packages must be developed to address the infor­ mation needs experienced by phc professionals, volunteers, parents and caregivers. the implementation of eci functions within the phc package may address some of the multiple problem areas in eci service delivery in south africa. the implementation of eci services in rural areas in south africa needs to be considered holistically, and the different programmes within the phc package should be used as a means to reach the communities. since the introduction of eci guidelines for speech-language therapists and audiologists in south africa by louw (1997), a theoretical framework for service delivery in the public sector has been described (kritzinger & louw, 2003). this framework pro­ vides a basis to assist in the planning of eci implementation. however, the potential of eci to prevent and minimise communi­ cation disabilities is not yet impacting on the south african health care system, especially in a rural district such as ditsobotla. the current research study indicated that difficulties may be ad­ dressed by means of integrating eci functions into the phc pack­ age. a diagrammatic representation of the proposed integration is provided in figure 2. e g entry level determine capacity of facilities, human resources and needs of community determine increments eci functions in primary prevention n o n-personal hea lth services promotion o f normal development -developmental surveillance planting increments disease maternal, child, hiv and m ental health school health prevention and wom an’s a 1 d s /s t i & substance & control health a n d t b abuse health monitoring & evaluation monitor increments implementing increments eci functions in secondary prevention disea se prevention and control providing eci and parent training planting increments i 1 m aternal, child hiv and m ental health and wom an’s aids /s t i & substance health health and tb abuse h ea lth m onitoring & evaluation monitor increments implementing increments determine increments eci functions in tertiary prevention disea se prevention and control rehabilitation o f disabilities planning increments 1 maternal, child h iv and m ental health and wom an’s a id s /s t i & substance health health and tb abuse hea lth m onitoring & evaluation monitor increments implementing increments figure 2. the incremental rollout of eci functions within the phc package the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 57 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) jeannie van der linde, alta kritzinger, aniel redelinghuys references babbie, e. (2004). the practice o f social research. (10th ed.). bel­ mont: wadsworth. de vos, a.s. (2005). combined quantitative and qualitative ap­ proach. in a.s. de vos (ed.). research at grass roots. for the social sciences and human service professions. (3rd ed.). (pp357-365) pretoria: van schaik. department of health (2000). the prim ary health care package for south africa a set o f norms and standards. pretoria: de­ partment of health. dennis, m. (2000). developmental plasticity in children: the role of biological risk, development, time and reserve. journal of communication disorders, 33:321-332. emmett, t. (2005). disability and poverty. in e. alant & l.l. lloyd (eds.). augm entative and alternative com m unication and se­ vere disabilities. beyond poverty. (pp68-94)london and phila­ delphia: whurr publishers. enderby, p. & petheram, b. (2000). an analysis of referrals to speech and language therapy in 11 centres, 1987-1995. inter­ national journal o f language and communication disorders, 35(2):137-146. equity project (2000). case study series: referrals from prim ary health care clinics in the eastern cape province. retrieved april, 2005 from http://www.doh.gov.za/docs/factsheets/ guidelines/dhp/part4g.pdf. escom. (2007) social impact assessment report. retrieved au­ gust 2009 from httd://www.escom.co.za/content/adpendix% 201 10%/20social%20assessment%20report.ddf. fair, l. & louw, b. (1999). early communication intervention within a community-based intervention model in south africa. the south african journal o f communication disorders, 46:13 23. fouche, c.b. & de vos, a.s. (2005). quantitative research de­ signs. in a.s. de vos (ed.). research at grass roots. for the social sciences and human service professions. (3rd ed.). (ppl32-143) pretoria: van schaik. greeff, m. (2002). information collection: interviewing. in a.s. de vos (ed.). research a t grass roots. for the social sciences and human service professions. (2nd ed.) (pp291— 320). pretoria: van schaik. kritzinger, a.m. (2000). establishing a com puter-based data sys­ tem for early communication intervention in south africa. un­ published doctoral thesis. pretoria: university of pretoria. kritzinger, a.m. & louw, l. (2003). clinical training of undergradu­ ate communication pathology students in neonatal assessment and neonate-caregiver interaction in south africa. the south african journal o f communication disorders, 50: 5-14. kritzinger, a., louw, b. & rossetti, l.m. (2001). a transdisciplinary conceptual framework for the early identification of risks for communication disorders in young children. the south african journal o f com m unication disorders, 48, 33-44. leedy, p.d. & ormrod, j.e. (2005). practical research: planning and design. (8th ed.). new jersey: pearson merrill prentice hall. lequerica, m. (1997). toward a one-stop model of service for low-lncome preschoolers: insights from clinical practice and research. infant-toddler intervention. the transdisciplinary journal, 7(4): 285-300. louw, b. (1997). early communication intervention: guidelines for speech-language therapists and audiologists. saslha: eth­ ics and standards committee. moodley, l. (1999). an in-service training program m e for com m u­ nity nurses in the identification o f at risk infants and toddlers. unpublished masters thesis. pretoria: university of pretoria. north west department of health. (2003). referral fram ework document. mafikeng. pickering, m., mcallister, l., hagler, p., whitehill, t.l., penn, c., robertson, s.j., & mccready, v. (1998). external factors influ­ encing the profession in six societies. american journal of speech-language pathology, 7, 4: 5-17. plante, e. & beeson, p. m. (2004). communication and com m uni­ cation disorders. a clinical introduction. (2nd ed.). boston: pear­ son. rossetti, l.m. (2001). com m unication intervention. birth to three. (2nd ed.). canada: singular thomson learning. steyn, a.g.w., smit, c.f., du toit, s.h.c., & strasheim, c. (1998). moderne statistiek vir die praktyk. (6th ed.). pretoria: van schaik. strydom, h. & venter, l. (2002). sampling and sampling meth­ ods. in a.s. de vos (ed.). research at grass roots. for the social sciences and human service professions. (2nd ed.) (pp 197 209). pretoria: van schaik. swanepoel, d.c.d. (2004). infant hearing screening at m aternal and child health clinics in a developing south african com m u­ nity. unpublished doctoral thesis. pretoria: university of preto­ ria. van rensburg, h.c.j. (2004). national health care systems: struc­ ture, types and dynamics. in h.c.j. van rensburg (ed.). health care in south africa, (ppl-5 0)pretoria: van schaik. werner, e. e. (2000). protective factors and individual resilience. in j. p. shonkoff, & s. j. meisels (eds.). handbook o f early childhood intervention (2nd ed.) (p p ll5 -1 3 2 ). london: cam­ bridge university press. 58 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) http://www.doh.gov.za/docs/factsheets/ http://www.escom.co.za/content/adpendix%25 the identification process in early communication intervention followed by primary health care personnel in ditsobotla sub-district appendix a rating scale for the description o f human and physical resources at clinics/hospitals n a m e o f c lin ic / h o sp ital w a r d ___________________________ p o p u latio n o f ch ild ren u n d er 5 y ears served b y this c lin ic /w a r d :________________ d o cto rs: n u m b er___________________ n u rs in g staff: n u m b er_______________ n u rs e s in train in g : n u m b e r__________ n u m b e r o f v isits p e r w ee k by: d o cto rs____________ s p eec h -lan g u a g e th e ra p ist & a u d io lo g is t_________ o cc u p atio n al th e r a p is t__________ d ietician __________ p h y sio th e ra p ist___________ p aed iatrician___________ o th er h ea lth care w o rk ers_____________ a re a s o f e v a lu a tio n l im ited a d eq u a te g ood c o m m en ts water supplywater supply at facility electricity provision o f electricity the average number o f infants the sister sees daily toys available for developmental screening general impression o f waiting rooms 1. seating for patients 2. posters on general health issues 3. posters on the development o f infants general impression o f consulting rooms 1. space for confidential interviews with parents/mothers 1 2. desk and chairs for phc personnel 3. medicine cabinets 4. posters on general health issues i 5. posters on the development o f infants general impression o f reception area 1. filing cabinets to ensure confidentiality o f patient files o the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 59 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) devising a developmental test of auditory perception: problems and prospects a r n o l d a b r a m o v i t z , m . s c . (psychol.) senior lecturer, department oj -psychology, university of cape town when, about six years ago, i embarked on the development of a psychological instrument that would evaluate certain complex auditory abilities in both normal and handicapped children, i knew i was undertaking an important, difficult, but not impossible, task. i still consider the task to be both important and difficult. but now, older, wiser and sadder, i no longer believe that a single individual, with a uni-disci plinary background, can cope with what such an undertaking entails. we will one day have a developmental test, or series of tests, that will provide genuine assistance in the multi-faceted problem of the assessment of auditory perception, but it will require the pooled resources of a multi-disciplinary team of workers—psychologists, linguists, phoneticians, audiologists, otologists, paediatricians, speech therapists, speech trainers, educationists, musicians, neurologists, physicists, sound engineers, statisticians, and other specialists impossible to specify in advance. why should this be? we have, after all, a developmental test of visual perception devised largely by a single person (frostig2). why should auditory perception prove to be such a different kettle of fish? what work i have done, of which some account is given in this paper, leads me irresistibly. to the conclusion that the reception, processing and interpretation of acoustic signals constitute some of the most elusive and complex phenomena to attempt to examine, gauge and measure. to a certain extent, this is due to the transient, ephemeral nature of sound itself. you cannot pin it down or "freeze" it in the way that you can, through drawings and pictures, capture and secure certain aspects of visual perception. so, correspondingly, you will find very few items in children's intelligence tests that are directly concerned with the basic skills related to auditory perception as such. the only exceptions 1 can think of are tests of auditory memory span, involving digits, words and sentences, which invoke a minimum of higher symbolic processes. psychologists have, in general, been quite shy about the developmental processes governing auditory perception, and have done very little in the gathering of relevant normative data. i am aware that various tests of auditory perception are used by speech and hearing pathologists, but the ones i have come across fail to meet certain minimum criteria which a general test of auditory abilities applicable tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 18, desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) arnold abramovitz 14 smile, or the other in which his demeanour is one of disappointment. three practice items are included. d. discrimination of the basic psychological parameters of formal tonal patterns, viz. pitch, loudness, duration and interval. twenty-four pairs of tone combinations form the basis of this test. three notes are used, c, d and ε (above middle c) recorded from the outputs of a series of sine-wave oscillators. the tonal patterns are random combinations of these three notes, using one of two levels of intensity and one of two periods of duration for each of the notes, and one of two intervals between the notes. six practice items are presented in which the different types of "error" are emphasized by the examiner whistling or singing the "tunes". three drawings are again used to assist in communicating the instructions. the first drawing shows a man and a boy each playing a bugle. the second and third drawings show the man respectively pleased and disappointed. their difficulty is increased (a) by increasing the number of notes per pattern (from three to five), (b) by introducing "errors" with respect to parameters which are more difficult to discriminate, e.g. intensity and duration, and (c) by decreasing the number (from four to one) of "errors" per pair. subjects this test was applied to 205 children, aged five to ten years, drawn from a middle-class, white, primary school population (the "nonhandicapped" sample), and to 232 white children with a variety of handicaps and difficulties (the "handicapped" sample). the latter included children from schools for (a) the deaf, (b) the hard-of-hearing, (c) the cerebral-palsied, (d) the retarded, (e) the blind, and (f) children referred to the u.c.t. child guidance clinic for various emotional, behavioural and scholastic problems. in addition to the test of auditory abilities described above, these children were assessed on the national bureau group test (either 5-6, 7-8 or 8-11 years, amended for individual administration), as well as the goodenough draw-a-man test and the digit-span test of the wisc. scoring-system finding a reliable scoring-system for the auditory abilities proved to be the major stumbling-block. relying on the test as a purely objective multiple-choice instrument, it turned out that for each of .the six agegroups, 5 years to 10 years, only the second sub-test, auditory figureground discrimination, possessed adequate split-half reliabilities. it was hoped that a scoring-system which .took into account aspects of the child's responses other than picture-matching would result in higher reliabilities. this was found to be quite feasible for the older, more intelligent child, where verbal and other responses to the sounds could be quantitatively scored or rated. it was, however, seldom possible to elicit these kinds of reactions from children of lower mental ages or journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. , december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental test of auditory perception: problems 15 children with communication handicaps. in the case of the first subtest, recognition of environmental sounds, it was found that a. threepoint scale (0, 1, 2) instead of a right-wrong criterion (1 or 0) resulted in somewhat larger odd-even correlation coefficients. the relevant quantitative data are tabulated elsewhere (abramovitz1)· results the reliabilities for recognition of environmental sounds were generally disappointing, especially at the 5-year and 10-year levels, but the corresponding standard errors of measurement, which roberts3 regards as more important than reliabilities for multiple-choice tests, were within reasonable limits. the reliabilities and standard errors of measurement for auditory figure-ground were uniformly good. it was decided, therefore, to proceed with the "handicapped" sample using these two sub-tests only—which could be administered in one session of 15-20 minutes. the next step was to establish the ability of these tests to discriminate between adjacent age groups. calculations of two-tailed levels of significance showed that the test in this form was not always suitable for accurate inter-age-level differentiations. this was not, however, the only objective of the study. having established the presence of a reasonably lawful developmental gradient, it was considered feasible to compile tentative norms for successive age levels. these were drawn up as quotient scores (via the calculation of percentile ranks), with 100 as the mean and 15 as the standard deviation. the quotient scores for the auditory tests were all reasonably close to those of the other ability tests, and it could be justifiably assumed that the auditory tests discriminated between different mental age levels to about the same degree as they did between corresponding chronological age levels. turning now to the "handicapped" groups, the first results to be considered are those for 19 subjects from a school for cerebral-palsied children, whose average age was about 10 years. although the average of their overall auditory quotients was not significantly different from that of their wisc quotients (about 80), the remarkable finding here was that mean figure-ground quotients were significantly lower (p=0,02) than single-sound quotients (75 vs. 91).-these subjects were not selected on the basis of any prior knowledge of their general perceptual abilities and in fact included children with purely motor handicaps. nevertheless, many children reported only one of each of the 20 double and triple sounds, a phenomenon which almost constituted a kind of negative auditory hallucination. this occurred in spite of the examiner's asking, or indicating by mime, very emphatically, for a more attentive reaction after each such failure. this was quite a dramatic phenomenon, and many of the trainers who were present during these sessions were clearly taken aback by the unexpected perfonnance of these children. they were about average when it came to identifying single environmental sounds, yet many were apparently unable to tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 18, desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) arnold abramovitz 16 establish the simultaneous presence of two sounds, each of which they had no difficulty with on its own. since t h e s e results pointed to more ominously defective auditory processes than seemed likely,· it was decided to test the hypothesis that they were simply due to perseveration arising from the previous presentation of 24 single sounds. sixteen of these subjects were retested four weeks later using slightly different instructions when presenting the first three double sounds (but in no other respect). if the subject failed to report both sounds, the examiner asked, "and what else did you hear?" (no extra points were awarded for correct responses arising from the new instruction on these three items). if this still failed to produce the correct response, the examiner would point out the two sounds concerned. after the third item no further help was given and the test was administered as before. the result of this simple change of instruction was quite as astonishing as the previous phenomenon. the average figure-ground quotients jumped from 78 to 99 (p=0,001). the average single sound quotients rose, non-significantly, from 92 to 97. the perseveration hypothesis was thus amply substantiated. this finding should not, however, be taken as having no bearing on these subjects' auditory abilities. many, if not most, real-life auditory situations do not allow for a "second chance". the auditory perseverator is a handicapped listener,, and his problem should not be minimized. after this experience, whenever there was a query about possible perseveration, the figure-ground test was always re-presented in toto, using the amended instructions described. the next group of subjects were taken from a home for retarded children, but their age-range extended from 7 years 6 months to 29 years 5 months, with a mean of 18 years 6 months. their iq's were not available, but were probably in the 25-60 region. each of the 45 children who could be tested (children below a mental age of about years could not) were also given a goodenough draw-a-man test. the results (using scores obtained from the amended instructions) showed (1) that mean figure-ground discrimination was significantly poorer (p=0,001) than single-sound. identification (61 vs. 72 months, test-age), (2) that the overall average auditory test-age was not significantly different from that for the goodenough draw-a-man test (66 vs. 68 months, test-age), and (3) that there were moderately high (0,45 to 0,64) positive correlations between these tests. sixteen children from a school for the deaf formed the 'next group of handicapped subjects. they were all tested (in the first instance) with hearing-aids fitted and adjusted, in the ordinary open-field manner adopted for hearing children. their average scores for single-sounds and figure-ground were almost identical, with a quotient-score of about 50. the question arose whether their hearing-aids were of real assistance to these children in this particular situation. it was hypothesized that, with the tape-recorder volume turned up to a subjectively comfortable listening level, and their hearing-aids removed, scores journal of the south african speech and • hearing association, vol. is, december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental test of auditory perception: problems 17 obtained would be higher because of the elimination of one source of distortion in the reproducing chain. ten of these children were re-tested a week later and a comparison of the results showed that highly significant increases occurred. although one could not be sure to what extent a practice effect contributed towards this improvement, a comparison with the brain-injured group is instructive. in the latter case the improvement in the single-sound scores was only about 6% and was not significant, whereas with the deaf group the improvement was about 17% and highly significant (p=0,001). the corresponding figure-ground improvements were about 14£% (p=0,001). in the absence of an adequate experimental design, inferences can only be tentative, but might be framed as follows: (a) deaf children do not ordinarily show the kind of perseveration that is seen with cerebralpalsied and retarded children, and (b) some deaf children are fitted with hearing-aids which amplify auditory stimuli at the cost of a significant degree of distortion. (it was a moving experience to find a profoundly deaf child with her unamplified ear glued to the loudspeaker coming up with scores that were at least as good as hearing children of her age-level.) the next group consisted of 16 children taken from a school for the hard-of-hearing. of these, six were randomly chosen for testing without their hearing aids. the ten hearing-aided children had mean single-sound and figure-ground quotients of 71 and 74 respectively, while the six unaided children had quotients of 76 and 83 respectively, a positive difference of 7% and 12% respectively. this shows roughly the same trend as the deaf sample, but not as markedly. the inference again is that peripheral hearing loss does not show the same pattern as organicity. (one boy, diagnosed as very severely "dyslexic", sailed through figure-ground without a single error—and without his aid. one couldn't help wondering where his real problem lay.) next was a sample of 58 children from two primary schools, whose principals queried their general school progress. in one case (a school serving parents in the professional and higher managerial class) there was a significantly lower mean figure-ground quotient (86) than intelligence quotient (109) (p=0,05). in the other case, however, (a school serving parents of the artisan and lower clerical class) there was no s u c h ^difference (97 in each case). teachers of the children from these two schools were asked to assess each child's reading and spelling achievement on a five-point scale: excellent—good—fair—rather weak —very poor. nineteen children were rated as "rather weak" or "very poof" on either or both reading and spelling. the average single-sound quotient and intelligence quotient were each 95, but the average figure-ground quotient was down to 81,5 (p=0,06). it was difficult to resist making the tentative inference of handicapped listening ability, due at least in part to some kind of auditory perseveration. the results for an unselected group of 32 children referred to the u.c.t. child guidance clinic for a variety of behavioural, emotional tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 18, desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) arnold abramovitz 18 and s c h o l a s t i c p r o b l e m s showed that although the mean figure-ground l p n , 92 was not much lower than the iq (94), it was significantly ?p=0 05) lower than the mean single-sounds quotient (102). fifteen children from this group were selected on the basis of a reading and/or spelling r e t a r d a t i o n of at least 12 months. although the mean figureground quotient (95) was lower than the mean single-sounds quotient (100), the difference was not significant. this finding does not correspond with that for a similar group taken from the two schools mentioned above. without further data and analysis there is no way of resolving the anomaly. finally, a group of 21 blind children were compared with an equal number of roughly equated sighted children (mean age for blind about 10 years, for sighted about 9 years, both groups of average intelligence). the test could obviously not be administered in the usual way, and instead of presenting four drawings, the examiner asked the subject (in both the blind and sighted groups) to choose from four spoken alternatives, after each auditory stimulus. mean quotient scores for the auditory tests were in all cases higher for the blind children. in the case of single-sounds this difference (106 vs. 99) was not statistically significant, but for figure-ground the difference (108 vs. 77) was significant at the 0,001 level. this result does not necessarily signify that blind children have superior auditory perceptual abilities of the kind tapped by this test, but rather that their retention of the recorded material and (in particular) the spoken alternatives was better than that of the sighted children, due to the intensive auditory training received at their institution—a great deal of which was being obtained through the medium of the tape-recorder. this last statement could be put the other way around, of course. the results might be seen as an indication of the comparative lack of training in listening skills which normal school-going children receive, and a corresponding bias (after a certain age-level) in favour of visual modes-of information-gathering. as with most individually-administered instruments, a great deal of information which eluded quantitative scoring could be gleaned about the responses of the subject to the test situation in general and the auditory stimuli in particular. there were apparently great individual differences in emotional and motivational reactions—from delight to boredom to aversion—as judged by facial expression gesture, posture, spontaneous vocalizations, verbalizations, and so on. the first subtest, recognition of environmental sounds, seemed to elifcit a pattern which might be called "alertness to the (non-linguistic) auditory environment", involving an ability to retain a sort of auditory after-image while the decision or discrimination between the four alternatives was made. this presumably took place by matching the auditory after-image with each of the auditory images aroused by the pictures, but perhaps not without a certain amount of deductive reasoning in some cases. the child with low scores here displays a variety of reactions, from an overconfident belief that he recognizes the sound before seeing the. pictures journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. , december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 19 developmental test of auditory perception: problems and an inability to decentrate (in the piagetian sense) when necessary; . to a°blank reaction ab initio and then either a "don't know" response (guessing was not encouraged); or a choice by the process of elimination; or a response-set for one of the four illustrations (the bottom left-hand drawing seemed to be a favourite for many handicapped children). one was often inclined to make the diagnosis of "auditory agnosia" when quotient scores on this test were very low, but this would of course be extremely rash in the absence of a great deal-of supporting d%ta. the figure-ground test provided the most compelling example of deviant auditory behaviour, namely the perseveration already referred to in its extreme form, it occurred only three times in the 205 nonhandicapped sample. the term "negative auditory hallucination" has already been mentioned in this connection, but the change in response as a result of slightly altered conditions of administration showed how inappropriate such an interpretation might be. this auditory perseveration occurred at all age-levels in the transition from double to triple sounds (for which no additional instructions were given with any of the groups). the test seems to reveal firstly the ability to focus on one of the multiple sounds at a time, i.e. perceiving it as "figure", whde ihe other sound or sounds rapidly and in turn are relegated to "ground this inability seemed likely in many cases to be part of a general personality factor, a sort of submissive, deferential reaction to the examiner in particular, and perhaps to the social environment in general. some of these children seemed even prepared to doubt the validity of their own experiences if they thought they were expected to make self-reports of a certain class and no other. a child's reaction to the test as a whole often gave the examiner a strong impression of "listening ability", or the lack of it, and this did not necessarily correspond to his score on the test, or to his intelligence results, or to the audiometrist's report of his hearing acuity. with regard to the latter issue, when a child of at least average intelligence was referred for scholastic, behavioural or emotional problems and scored below a quotient of 85 on the auditory test as a whole, audiometry was recommended and in most cases carried out. the resulting audiograms were mostly within normal limits but at least 6 children were found to have slight (15-25 db) conductive losses and at least 3 children were found to have moderate (25-35 db) conductive losses, in at least one ear correspondingly, some of the children from the school for the hard-of-hearing (and even one from school for the deaf) had average or above-average auditory quotients. in spite of some of the interesting and useful group results obtained, the test as used was a truncated and relatively unrefined instrument, not really suitable for reliable individual assessment. this being so, what could be done to improve the reliability and validity of the two sub-tests used, and what sub-tests could be devised to replace the abandoned speech sound discrimination and tonal pattern discrimination tests? i think these are questions for our imaginary multitydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 18 desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) arnold abramovitz 20 disciplinary team to thrash out, but some of my own thoughts may not be out of place. _ as to the first question, i think the basic notions behind the two sub-tests used are reasonably sound. what is needed is to mount a full-scale programme in which a much larger number of subjects of diverse backgrounds' serve in the pilot runs and finally in the standardization sample. using a larger number and variety of sounds and matching pictures, those which don:t contribute to the reliability and validity of the test can be eliminated. with regard to' the second question, i must first of all ruefully admit to wanting to leave speech sound discrimination to the exclusive jurisdiction of speech and hearing specialists. this whole territory is a veritable minefield for the innocent psychologist. consider, for example, the almost impossible task of constructing a comprehensive test which, in addition to meeting the criteria already alluded to, will not favour one linguistic community over another. in connection with tonal pattern recognition, i now consider that the fourth criterion is misconceived, and that for this particular kind of skill, at least, one should not try to isolate the auditory modality from the visual and the kinaesthetic. all perception, as taylor's4 compelling work reminds us, is intersensory, and we specifically need tests of audiovisual, visuo-auditory and audiomotor skills. such tests exist of course. how suitable they are for the purposes of constructing an all-embracing developmental test of auditory perception remains an issue for debate and research. finally, a technical point, but a very important one, which applies to every kind of auditory test-stimulus. convenient as the ordinary taperecorder undoubtedly is (compared, say, to disc recordings), it has an inherent disadvantage. you cannot re-present a stimulus without a good deal of fuss and delay. what is needed is a tape-player which allows the examiner to re-administer a given item as often as he wishes with no waste of time. there is at least one such instrument on the market that i know of, namely the bell & howell "language master" (probably familiar to most readers of this journal) in which cards of varying dimensions carrying the magnetic tape are conveniently fed into the machine by hand. these cards could also have, behind them, the multiple-choice pictures, and the subject's score would depend, in part, on the number of times the item had to be presented before the correct picture was pointed to. there is an electronic-acoustic problem to be solved, namely altering the recording-head and circuit to provide an adequate frequency response, say 4 0 15khz + 3db. summary it is certain that many children whose auditory perception is queried by audiologists, speech therapists, educationists and psychologists elude the diagnostic screens presently available in each of these disciplines. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. , december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) developmental test of auditory perception: problems 21 the need for a qualitative and quantitative psychological assessment of the child's auditory abilities and disabilities led to the development of a test which was intended to evaluate the following functions: (a) recognition of environmental sounds, (b) auditory figure-ground discrimination, (c) speech-sound discrimination (phonemic and mtonational) and (d)-tonal pattern discrimination (pitch, loudness, duration and interval). it was not intended to investigate threshold phenomena as such but rather to supplement and complement pure-tone and speech audiometry. the test was applied to 205 children, aged five to ten years, drawn from a normal school population, and 232 children with difficulties and handicaps varying both in degree and kind. only the first two sub-tests were found to be clinically and experimentally viable, and data for the curtailed test are presented. the following results are noteworthy: (1) the test measures functions which are positively related to both age and intelligence. (2) brain-injured, retarded and emotionally disturbed children generally test low on auditory figure-ground discrimination; this vulnerability is most likely due to perseveration. (3) previously unsuspected peripheral hearing losses may sometimes be detected by the use of the test. on the other hand, some children said to have high degrees of hearing loss test at or above their age-level. (4) many deaf and hard-of-hearing children test higher without their hearing-aids; this is probably due to amplification being achieved at the cost of distortion. (5) children of average intelligence with reading and/or spelling difficulties often test low on auditory figure-ground discrimination. (6) blind children who have received auditory training are equal to sighted children in recognition of environmental sounds, but superior in auditory figure-ground discrimination. this does not, however, necessarily signify superior auditory perception as such on the part of the blind. r .. in general it is concluded that the development of tests of auditory perception could add significantly to the psycho-educational assessment of both "normal" and handicapped children. ο opsomming daar kan met sekerheid aanvaar word dat baie kinders van wie die ouditiewe waarnemingsvermoe betwyfel word deur oudioloe, spraakterapeute, opvoedkundiges en sielkundiges, die diagnostiese toetse vandag tot hul beskikking ontglip. die behoefte wat daar bestaan vir 'n kwalitatiewe en kwantitatiewe sielkundige bepaling van 'n kind se ouditiewe vermoe en onvermoe het gelei tot die ontwikkeling van 'n toets wat gemik is op die evaluasie van die volgeiide funksies: 1. herkenning van omgewingsgeluide. 2. ouditiewe figuur-grond onderskeiding. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 18, desember 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22" arnold abramovitz 3. spraakklank diskriminasie (foneem sowel as intonasie-onderskeiding), en "4.: tonale patroondiskriminasie (toonhoogte, luidheid, duur , en interval). . dit was nie die bedoeling om drempelverskynsels te ondersoek me, maar slegs om 'n toets te ontwikk'el wat ter aanvulling kan dien by suiwertoonen spraakoudiometrie. die toets is uitgevoer op 205 kinders, met ouderdomme wat wissel van 5 tot 10 jaar, getrek uit 'n bevolking van normale skoolkinders en op 232 kinders met afwykings en belemmerings wat verskil betreffende die tipe afwyking asook die erns van die probleem. slegs die eerste twee subtoetse is klinies en eksperimenteel uitvoerbaar en data vir die verkorte toets word uiteen gesit. die volgende resultate kan beslis op gelet word: 1. die toets meet funksies wat in verhouding staan tot beide ouderdom en intelligensie. 2. breinbeskadigde, vertraagde en emosioneel-versteurde kinders gee 'n lae telling by toetsing van ouditiewe figuur-grond onderskeiding; hierdie kwetsbaarheid is moontlik toe te skryf aan perseverasie. 3. 'n perifere gehoorverlies voorheen onopgemerk, kan soms met behulp van hierdie toets opgespoor word. aan die anderkant weer toets sommige kinders van wie gese is dat hul hoe grade van gehoorverlies het, by of bokant hul ouderdomspeile. 4. baie dowe en hardhorende kinders toets hoer sonder hul gehoor apparate; dit is moontlik tewyte aan versterking verkry ten koste van distorsie. 5. kinders van gemiddelde intelligensie met leesen/of spelprobleme gee dikwels 'n lae telling by toetsing van ouditiewe figuur-grond onderskeiding. 6. blinde kinders wat reeds getiooropleiding ontvang het, behaal dieselfde resultate as siende kinders wat herkenning van omgewingsgeluide betref, maar is superieur wat ouditiewe figuur-grond onderskeiding betref; hierdie resultate dui nie noodwendig op superieure ouditiewe persepsie by die blinde nie. daar word ten slotte beweer dat die ontwikkeling van ouditiewe persepsietoetse baie kan bydra tot die psigo-opvoedkiyidige toetsing van beide „normale" en gestremde kinders. references / 1 abramovitz, a. (1967): auditory perception and the handicapped child. paper presented at the xviii annual congress of the s.a. psychological association, cape town, september 1967. (unpublished mimeograph). .2 frostig, m. et al. (1964): developmental test of visual perception. perceptual and motor skills, 19, 463-499. 3. roberts, a. d. h. (1962): the maximum reliability of a multiple-choice test. psychologia africana, 9, 286-293. 4. taylor, j. g. (1962): the behavioural basis of perception. yale university press, new haven, connecticut. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 18, december 1971 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abstract introduction research methodology and design results discussion ethical considerations conclusion acknowledgements references about the author(s) mckinley andrews discipline of speech-language pathology, university of kwazulu-natal, south africa mershen pillay discipline of speech-language pathology, university of kwazulu-natal, south africa citation andrews, m., & pillay, m. (2017). poor consistency in evaluating south african adults with neurogenic dysphagia. south african journal of communication disorders 64(1), a158. https://doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v64i1.158 original research poor consistency in evaluating south african adults with neurogenic dysphagia mckinley andrews, mershen pillay received: 25 mar. 2016; accepted: 11 sept. 2016; published: 23 jan. 2017 copyright: © 2017. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract background: speech-language therapists are specifically trained in clinically evaluating swallowing in adults with acute stroke. incidence of dysphagia following acute stroke is high in south africa, and health implications can be fatal, making optimal management of this patient population crucial. however, despite training and guidelines for best practice in clinically evaluating swallowing in adults with acute stroke, there are low levels of consistency in these practice patterns. objective: the aim was to explore the clinical practice activities of speech-language therapists in the clinical evaluation of swallowing in adults with acute stroke. practice activities reviewed included the use and consistency of clinical components and resources utilised. clinical components were the individual elements evaluated in the clinical evaluation of swallowing (e.g. lip seal, vocal quality, etc.) methods: the questionnaire used in the study was replicated and adapted from a study increasing contentand criterion-related validity. a narrative literature review determined what practice patterns existed in the clinical evaluation of swallowing in adults. a pilot study was conducted to increase validity and reliability. purposive sampling was used by sending a self-administered, electronic questionnaire to members of the south african speech-language-hearing association. thirty-eight participants took part in the study. descriptive statistics were used to analyse the data and the small qualitative component was subjected to textual analysis. results: there was high frequency of use of 41% of the clinical components in more than 90% of participants (n = 38). less than 50% of participants frequently assessed sensory function and gag reflex and used pulse oximetry, cervical auscultation and indirect laryngoscopy. approximately a third of participants showed high (30.8%), moderate (35.9%) and poor (33.3%) consistency of practice each. nurses, food and liquids and medical consumables were used usually and always by more than 90% of participants. conclusion: infrequent use of clinical components and high variability in clinical practice among speech-language therapists calls for uniform curricula in the clinical evaluation of swallowing at south african universities and for continued professional development post-graduation. different contexts and patient symptoms contribute towards varied practice; however, there is still a need to improve consistency of practice for quality health care delivery. a research-based policy for the clinical swallowing evaluation for a resource-limited context is also needed. introduction the focus of this study was on practice patterns used by speech-language therapists in the clinical swallow evaluation of neurogenic dysphagia in adults with acute stroke. practice patterns are located relative to the ‘curriculum of practice’ (figure 1) and are defined as consistent activities that have widely led to improved health outcomes (carnaby & harenberg, 2013; mathers-schmidt & kurlinski, 2003). this is important for quality of health care and adequate standards of practice (carnaby & harenberg, 2013). practice is also defined as activity related to and influenced by theoretical knowledge, education and training (pillay, kathard & samuel, 1997). knowledge is moulded by social, political, gender, ethnic, cultural and economic influences (pillay et al., 1997), and as these influences play large roles in the south african context these will most likely impact speech-language therapists’ practice. policies are broad practice guidelines based on recent literature, providing the speech-language therapist with general advice on what to do in clinical procedures and how to achieve them (pillay et al., 1997). clinical practice activities are those procedures a professional performs and the resources they use so as to manage an adult with neurogenic dysphagia (pillay et al., 1997). this includes assessing clinical components in the clinical swallow evaluation. clinical components are the individual elements that make up the clinical swallow evaluation for which the speech-language therapist is responsible for evaluating. clinical practice is the procedure or protocol that is followed by all health professionals who manage adults with stroke (davis & taylor-vaisey, 1997; heinemann et al., 2003; van peppen, hendriks, meeteren, helders & kwakkel, 2007). this knowledge–theory relationship gives rise to evidence-based practice which is an effective and high-quality practice that is driven by knowledge from the latest research (american speech-language-hearing association, 2005) as well as by the speech-language therapist’s competency, experience and preference for practice (riquelme, 2015). evidence-based practice is a crucial part of health care delivery as it contributes towards improving health outcomes for those with neurogenic dysphagia due to acute stroke (straus, tetroe & graham, 2009). figure 1: the curriculum of practice. a narrative review of the literature on practice patterns of speech-language therapists during the clinical swallow evaluation of adults with neurogenic dysphagia post-acute stroke was performed. a limited amount of literature was available. all 12 studies included in the study showed that most clinical swallow evaluations comprise four main subsections: history, oral motor examination, voice and trial swallows; however, more specific elements still differ among speech-language therapists and have not been officially or extensively outlined by the literature (riquelme, 2015). key findings showed that clinical components utilised by more than 90% of speech-language therapists ranged between 24% and 63% across five studies, and the consistency of clinical component utilisation varied between 32% and 58% across three studies. there were infrequent assessments of the gag reflex, sensation and mental status as well as limited use of indirect laryngoscopy, pulse oximetry and cervical auscultation methods. little information was found on what resources are used during the clinical swallowing evaluation. mathers-schmidt and kurlinski (2003), bateman, leslie and drinnan (2007) and pettigrew and o’toole (2007) observed practice patterns of speech-language therapists in the usa, ireland and the uk. infrequent use of clinical components and low levels of consistent use of clinical components in the clinical swallow evaluation were reported in all three studies. carnaby and harenberg (2013) also found high variability of practice patterns among speech-language therapists in dysphagia evaluation. despite receiving both policies guiding clinical practice and relevant education and training, practice still seems to vary internationally among speech-language therapists. contextual and patient-specific factors will expectedly play a role in differing practice; however, it is expected that higher degrees of clinical components are utilised according to protocols and practice guidelines to ensure a certain standard of optimal health care and evidence-based practice. the occurrence of stroke and its resulting comorbidities continues to be highly prevalent in south africa. stroke is the second highest cause of death in the world and annually claims approximately five million lives (sajjad et al., 2013). over four million of these deaths occur in lowand middle-income countries, such as south africa (sajjad et al., 2013). stroke was the fourth leading cause of mortality in south africa after (1) tuberculosis, (2) influenza and pneumonia and (3) aids and was the leading reason for death in adults above the age of 65 years in 2013 (statistics south africa, 2014). about 67 000 of south africa’s population experience a stroke annually (maredza, bertram & tollman, 2015). dysphagia is a common symptom post-stroke and results in increased mortality and morbidity rates, poor nutrition and dehydration, prolonged disability, and decreased quality of life (gonzález-fonández, ottenstein, atanelov & christian, 2013b; guyomard et al., 2009). up to 50% of adults with stroke are at risk of aspirating and developing pneumonia (martino et al., 2005). the management of dysphagia post-stroke is therefore important for optimal health care. south africa faces a quadruple burden of disease which is aggravated by poverty, high levels of unemployment, socio-economic inequity and an ineffective health system (mayosi et al., 2012). the four epidemics include hiv and tuberculosis, non-communicable diseases and mental health disorders, deaths related to injury and violence and maternal, neonatal and child mortalities (mayosi et al., 2012). south african speech-language therapists are therefore often faced with adults with stroke who have additional diseases (mayosi et al., 2012) that complicate management and prognosis. the south african context can thus be a complex and challenging one for the speech-language therapist. dysphagia resulting from acute stroke is managed by speech-language therapists as it is within their scope of practice to conduct a clinical swallow evaluation. the health professions council of south africa (hpcsa) (2009) stated that the speech-language therapist’s scope of practice is determined by the level of their education, experience and skill. it also stated that services provided must be evidence-based and culturally and linguistically appropriate for the adult with neurogenic dysphagia. the speech-language therapist therefore needs to be up-to-date with clinical swallow evaluation guidelines and recent research in such practices. speech-language therapists fulfil this scope of practice by initially using the clinical swallow evaluation, which is a non-instrumental, behavioural assessment procedure involving identifying and interpreting various components of information from the patient, family and various health professionals. the primary goals are to conclude the presence, nature and cause of the dysphagia, and to determine the level of dysfunction, the risk for aspiration and whether nutritional status is adversely affected (pettigrew & o’toole, 2007). the result of such an evaluation aids the development of an appropriate swallowing management plan (gonzález-fonández et al., 2013b). due to the subjective nature of the clinical swallow evaluation and the grave complications dysphagia may cause it is vital for the speech-language therapist to be informed of the relevant knowledge and skills regarding the clinical swallow evaluation. mccullough et al. (2005) reported that when an adult with neurogenic dysphagia aspirates, silent aspiration occurs half of the time. silent aspiration also makes the speech-language therapist’s recent knowledge and skills important when performing a clinical swallow evaluation. silent aspiration occurs when a bolus is aspirated (enters the trachea below the level of the true vocal folds), but no cough reflex is produced as a result (smith hammond & goldstein, 2006). in south africa, because the dysphagia evaluation faces many challenges, as discussed below, it is thought that performing a thorough and optimal clinical swallow evaluation can be difficult. not only is there a dire shortage of trained and skilled health professionals involved in dysphagia management such as doctors and nurses (department of health, 2011; george, quinlan & reardon, 2009; mayosi et al., 2012), but they are also unequally distributed between urban and rural areas (mills et al., 2011). in 2011, south africa had 7.7 doctors and 40.8 nurses and midwives per 10 000 people in the country (department of health, 2011). in sub-saharan africa this is exacerbated by hiv, migration of staff to developed countries and a shortage of training institutions to train adequate amounts of health professionals (mills et al., 2011). such a shortage of health professionals makes obtaining information about the nature and history of the swallow more challenging, for example obtaining temperature records and a feeding history from a nurse, information from a radiologist regarding the location of brain insult as well as chest status or c-reactive protein information from doctors. there is also a shortage of medical facilities and reduced standard of care due to poor infrastructure and support (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010; mayosi et al., 2012; national development plan, 2012). south africa is culturally and economically diverse. the use of traditional medicine in south africa is a large trade, and for some living in rural areas it is their only option for health care (mander, ntuli, diederichs & mavundla, 2007). however, some prefer the treatment from traditional healers (called sangomas) to western medicine, and the speech-language therapist should consider their influence in the clinical swallow evaluation (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010). for example traditional healers may have recommended that specific thin fluids be consumed that in fact are not safe to swallow for an adult with neurogenic dysphagia. knowing such information is valuable during the clinical swallowing evaluation as it contributes information regarding the consistency of liquids the adult is expected to swallow at home. there are 11 official languages in south africa which often leads to difficulty in communicating effectively with the adult with stroke and their caregivers (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010). this makes identifying accurate swallowing history and background information from the caregivers and the adult with neurogenic dysphagia problematic. the adult with neurogenic dysphagia’s ability to follow the speech-language therapist’s instructions during the evaluation also gets adversely affected by such language barriers. the speech-language therapist must consider all these influential factors discussed during the clinical swallow evaluation in the south african context. there was a need to investigate the protocol that south african speech-language therapists follow during the clinical swallow evaluation of adults with neurogenic dysphagia post-acute stroke, as currently such practice is unknown. due to the life-threatening nature of dysphagia and the various challenges encountered in the south african context, it is important to explore what practice currently exists and whether it is of adequate standard to ensure safe and optimal management of those who have suffered acute stroke and resulting in dysphagia. the results of this study will ultimately contribute towards a uniform education and training curriculum for the clinical swallow evaluation among south african universities as well as the development of uniform guidelines and policies for such practices. any poor adherence to existing practice guidelines and clinical component utilisation may contribute towards motivating for uniform curricula. by providing uniform curricula, the quality of training is thought to improve, or at least generalise better clinical practice by increasing levels of preparedness for evaluating dysphagia (singh et al., 2015). aims and objectives the aim of the study was to explore the practice patterns that south african speech-language therapists follow in the clinical swallowing evaluation of adults with neurogenic dysphagia due to acute stroke. objectives included exploring practice patterns in terms of: the frequency of clinical component utilisation, the consistency of clinical component utilisation, the frequency of resources used and the factors contributing towards infrequent use of resources. research methodology and design a survey design was used for the study and exploratory and descriptive designs were also incorporated. a survey design is considered a valuable method of obtaining original data to describe a large population that cannot be observed directly, or are difficult to observe (leedy & ormrod, 2013). the study explored and obtained information and more understanding about practice patterns as they have not yet been fully researched (bless, higson-smith & kagee, 2006). the researcher performed a narrative review of the literature to inform the adaptation of a questionnaire from a study by bateman et al. (2007). a narrative review is a thorough review of published literature on a specific topic. it has been known to consist of relatively unsystematic methods, but is a good summary of up-to-date information (green, johnson & adams, 2006). the study was replicated and based on a questionnaire taken and adapted from a study by bateman et al. (2007), and thus all its clinical components were replicated and included in the current study. further modifications from a pilot study were applied to the questionnaire to improve its content and layout and overall feasibility (table 1). eight participants returned a feedback sheet electronically reporting questionnaire adaptations. participants had 2 weeks over august and september 2015 to participate in the pilot study. the south african speech-language-hearing association (saslha) recruited participants by e-mail and via the social media website facebook®. participants accessed the self-administered, electronic questionnaire on the web survey development company website survey monkey® over a time frame of 4 weeks in october and november 2015. table 1: description and rationale for the pilot study adaptations to the questionnaire. participants were selected via purposive sampling. only people with specific knowledge and skills were invited to take part, with the purpose of being able to provide data competently for the study (leedy & ormrod, 2013). training with regularly working with adults with neurogenic dysphagia enables one to be proficient in assessing this population and thus to provide valid data for the study (hpcsa, 2009). participants were practitioners who had a speech-language therapy degree in 2014 or earlier. the participants were currently working with adults with neurogenic dysphagia post-acute stroke in south africa. these participants, therefore, had sufficient and recent experience and knowledge with this population (hpcsa, 2009). a total of 38 participants completed the electronic questionnaire. babbie and mouton (2001) state that 5–25 participants in the interpretive paradigm is a sufficient sample size. most of the participants in the study (n = 17; 44.7%) had between 1 and 5 years of experience both as a speech-language therapist and working with adults with neurogenic dysphagia, and 23.6% (n = 9) of the participants were completing their community service year (first year of work) (figure 2). figure 2: biographical information of participants (n = 38): (a) years working as a speech-language therapist; (b) years working with adult neurogenic dysphagia; (c) number of adults with neurogenic dysphagia evaluated in a typical month; and (d) percentage of caseload of adults with neurogenic dysphagia in the last year. descriptive statistics were used to analyse quantitative data. raw data were converted into numerical data via coding (babbie & mouton, 2001). the frequency of clinical component and resource usage was determined by converting the frequency of codes into percentages (babbie & mouton, 2001). the consistency of clinical component use was calculated as how many clinical components were used with the same frequency across participants (bateman et al., 2007). those clinical components used with the same frequency by more than 75% of participants were grouped as highly consistent, those between 50% and 75% as moderately consistent and those below 50% as inconsistent (bateman et al., 2007). the small qualitative component was subjected to textual analysis, where themes were identified in the text and coded into numerical values (leedy & ormrod, 2013). the frequency of occurrence of themes was identified by counting how many times each code was recorded, and the themes were discussed (leedy & ormrod, 2013). results clinical components in the clinical swallow evaluation the frequency of use of clinical components by participants forty-one per cent of the clinical components (16/39) were usually or always used by more than 90% of the participants (n = 38). these included the following: medical history (100%); language abilities or cognitive communication abilities (100%); overall oral efficiency (100%); vocal quality preand post-swallow (100%); oral residue (100%); overall opinion of airway safety (100%); medical status (97.4%); variety of bolus types (97.4%); lip seal (97.3%); oral structures, muscles and functioning (97.3%); vocal quality (94.8%); nutritional status (94.7%); laryngeal elevation (94.7%); saliva control/management (94.7%); respiratory status (92.1%); and patient interview/perception of the problem (92.1%). forty-six per cent of clinical components (18/39) were usually or always utilised by a range of 50–90% of the participants, and 12.8% (5/39) were usually or always utilised by less than 50% (0% to 31.6%) of the participants. the consistency of use of clinical components by participants there was a high consistency of clinical practice among participants for 12 out of 39 clinical components (30.8%) (figure 3). these clinical components included the following: medical history (89.5%); medical status (89.5%); oral structures, muscles and functioning (86.8%); indirect laryngoscopy (86.8%); lip seal (84.2%); laryngeal elevation (84.2); vocal quality preand post-swallow (84.2); overall oral efficiency (81.6%); pharyngeal swallow initiation delay (81.6%); overall opinion of airway safety (81.6%); saliva control/management (76.3%); and oral residue (76.3%). participants showed a high consistency of practice by always using these clinical components, with the exception of indirect laryngoscopy, which was never used by more than 75% of the participants. although these clinical components were used highly consistently, no clinical components reached more than 90% consistency. figure 3: consistency of clinical component use. there were high rates of variability of clinical practice among the remaining participants (69.2%). clinical practice was moderately consistent for 35.9% of the clinical components (14/39) and inconsistent for the remaining 33.3% (13/39). these included evaluating head and neck control and posture (44.7%); trials with compensatory techniques (44.7%); various features of speech (39–42%); visual, auditory and/or motor abilities (34.2%); and sensory function (34.2%). the clinical components used least consistently included medication use, social history, background information and gag reflex (31.6%). resources in the clinical swallowing evaluation physical resources on average 37.9% of responses showed that physical resources (both medical and non-medical) were used with low frequencies in the clinical swallow evaluation (table 2). physical resources included consumables, medical instruments, food and liquid items, eating utensils and food and liquid modifying agents. more than half of the responses (57.3%) indicated limited access to physical resources such as medical instruments, adaptive eating utensils and food and liquid modifying agents. other reasons for using physical resources infrequently included patient-specific needs, theft and limited time, limited staff and limited funding. also, it was mentioned that other health professionals use certain resources instead; for example, occupational therapists issue adapted spoons. table 2: frequency and consistency of clinical components and resources utilised in the clinical swallow assessment by participants. medical resources: more than 90% of participants used consumable resources (e.g. gloves, tongue depressors) usually and always in the clinical swallow evaluation; however, over half (57.9%) of participants never and seldom used medical instruments such as stethoscopes and pulse oximeters. fourteen per cent of responses conveyed that little or no training with medical instruments was received, nor were there opportunities for experience with them. participants expressed a desire for such training as well as for mentoring from experienced clinicians specifically as they felt unprepared and uncomfortable in the work place. non-medical resources: more than 90% of participants used food and liquid items usually and always in clinical swallow evaluation, but only 36.9% usually and always used eating utensils. this may have been because examples given to participants were of adaptive eating utensils (e.g. dysphagia cups) and not typical ones (e.g. spoons and forks). participants may therefore have reported on the use of only adaptive eating utensils, not eating utensils in general, including typical eating utensils. just over half of participants (55.5%) usually and always used food and liquid modifying agents in the clinical swallow evaluation. human resources over 90% of participants consulted with nurses usually and always, and 86.8% of participants consulted with doctors and allied health professionals like physiotherapists and occupational therapists usually and always in the clinical swallow evaluation. it was reported that nurses were required for their knowledge regarding the patient’s feeding practices. occupational therapists and physiotherapists were consulted when evaluating head and neck control, positioning and visual and motor abilities. physiotherapists also provided information regarding the lungs while occupational therapists assisted with adaptive eating utensils. other human resources reported to be in the clinical swallow evaluation were ear, nose and throat specialists, neurologists, social workers, caregivers or family members and dieticians who confirmed whether the patient was malnourished and assisted with food and liquid consistency modifications. three participants also reported the use of a medical rehabilitation team. eleven per cent of responses showed that human resources like doctors and nurses were used with low frequencies in the clinical swallow evaluation. participants reported that human resources were simply not available. in summary, nurses, food and liquids and medical consumables were used usually and always by more than 90% of participants. almost a third (29.1%) of responses showed infrequent (never, seldom and half the time) use of all resources on average (table 2). discussion clinical components in the clinical swallow evaluation the frequency of use of clinical components by participants all those clinical components used frequently (41%) in the clinical swallow examination indicated evidence-based practice by participants, as these clinical components are all supported by the literature, are included in policies and in training and thus it is expected that they are frequently included (american speech-language-hearing association, 2004; bateman et al., 2007). the remaining 23 clinical components (59%) were used infrequently and are discussed here. fifty-eight per cent of participants identified medication use usually and always. this might have been due to poor education and training after graduation, or challenging circumstances in which to obtain such information. this remains, however, important information to identify in the clinical swallow evaluation as many types of medication can adversely influence dysphagia, such as anti-depressants, medication for blood pressure and for nausea, which can all cause xerostomia (balzer, 2000). these may need to be treated after receiving a stroke. any medicine that depresses the central nervous system can limit sensation, awareness and voluntary muscle control (balzer, 2000). even the act of swallowing medication can be a hazard in itself (schiele et al., 2015). it is important that the speech-language therapist not only identify which medication has been prescribed but also in which form the adult with neurogenic dysphagia is consuming it. general background information (education, vocational, socio-economic and cultural information) and social history were identified by 57.9% of participants usually and always. this important information is to aid decisions regarding what is assessed in the clinical swallow evaluation and how to go about planning management. one participant mentioned the importance of considering modified diets in accordance with cultural preferences. it is the speech-language therapist’s responsibility to conduct practice that is socially and culturally sensitive, especially in south africa where the population is culturally and linguistically diverse. considering culture and diversity consists of more than race and ethnicity and is often overlooked. it includes considering language, religion, customs, values, tastes, lifestyle, education, profession and age, among others. this reduces bias, endorses cultural understanding in practice and puts the adult at ease and increases compliance with the speech-language therapist (riquelme, 2013). it is also important to understand the adult’s beliefs with regard to matters such as death and certain medical procedures, such as nasogastric tube or percutaneous endoscopic gastrostomy insertion. other allied health professionals, such as physiotherapists and occupational therapists, are also expected to consider cultural differences in their practice (riquelme, 2007). difficulty in communicating, difference in language and/or poor availability of caregivers who can provide background information are often reasons for not assessing this information. limited and unreliable public transport services in south africa or the inability of the caregivers to afford transport are often reasons for their inability to get to hospital. riquelme (2007) recommends using a professional interpreter for language barriers because colleagues or family members may not disclose complete information due to cultural privacy beliefs and credibility of the message may be lost. such human resources are often not available in south africa posing a challenge for the speech-language therapist to adequately obtain accurate information in the clinical swallow evaluation. vocal prosody was evaluated by 57.9% of speech-language therapists usually and always. it provides information on the ability of the patient to control aspects such as intonation, rhythm and stress in their speech and can give clues as to whether dysarthria is present or not and which type of dysarthria it may be. the identification of dysarthria aids decisions regarding the presence of aspiration and gives clues to the nature of dysphagia as it can be predictive of aspiration (schroeder, daniels, mcclain, corey & foundas, 2006). it was perhaps not assessed as much because other speech characteristics provide them with more information regarding dysarthria or aspiration. there may also be poor education and training after graduation. some participants mentioned that a speech function assessment was performed informally, but was not always completed if the patient was medically unstable or due to time constraints where assessing swallowing function took priority. fifty-five per cent of participants identified visual, auditory and/or motor abilities frequently. these give the speech-language therapist an idea of the patient’s abilities and limitations. motor difficulties can affect the patient’s oral musculature and movements and goes hand-in-hand with safe head and trunk positioning for swallowing (american speech-language-hearing association, 2004; gonzález-fonández et al., 2013a). appropriate positioning is an integral factor in terms of safe swallowing. vision can affect feeding abilities and the ability to hear can affect the following of instructions. given the key role that sensation plays in dysphagia, it is concerning that sensation was often evaluated by only 31.6% of participants. mathers-schmidt and kurlinski (2003), bateman et al. (2007) and pettigrew and o’toole (2007) reported higher frequencies of utilisation of this clinical component, which were 74.2%, 56% and 76%, respectively. sensory feedback helps the triggering of the swallow, chewing and salivary flow and is critical for effective swallowing (rogers & arvedson, 2005). reasons for such poor utilisation of sensation in the clinical swallow evaluation are possibly due to limited resources such as sour, sweet and bitter bolus variations or varying temperature boluses. there are also no clear guidelines or satisfactory measurement techniques for interpretation of oral sensitivity testing (pettigrew & o’toole, 2007). it is also interesting to note that south african policies and guidelines do not delineate assessing sensory function in the clinical swallow evaluation. it may also be that speech-language therapists have forgotten how important sensation is in the swallowing process, indicating how continued education after graduating is so important. it was noted that the more experienced speech-language therapists were more likely to evaluate sensation than those currently completing their community service. this is surprising as those speech-language therapists currently completing their community service have recently studied and thus have knowledge and theory that is more up-to-date. this highlights the importance of both clinical experience and continued professional education. only 18.5% of participants usually and always used pulse oximetry methods, and as many as 63.2% of participants never and seldom used it. the reliability of using pulse oximetry to detect aspiration in the clinical swallow evaluation has received conflicting support in the literature (chong, lieu, sitoh, meng & leow, 2003; ramsey et al., 2003), and thus it seems speech-language therapists decide for themselves whether it should be utilised (bateman et al., 2007). this may indicate that speech-language therapists are acknowledging the varying support in the literature and using it according to their judgement in the clinical swallow evaluation, thus perhaps successfully translating knowledge into practice. other reasons for poor utilisation included poor training and poor availability of pulse oximeters (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010) to monitor the oxygen saturation levels in arterial blood during the act of swallowing. as many as 50% of participants never or seldom utilised gag reflex testing, and only 18.4% of participants assessed it frequently. there is varying clinical usefulness of this clinical component in the literature, and thus it seems speech-language therapists are using it as they see fit (mccullough et al., 2005; oliveira et al., 2015). other reasons for poor use were that it was uncomfortable for the patient and that the doctor assessed it. only 7.9% of participants usually and always used cervical auscultation procedures, while 81.6% participants never and seldom used it. this was mainly due to poor training and limited availability of medical equipment. there are conflicting results of the reliability of such a method in the literature, but cervical auscultation has been shown to have generally good input towards the overall picture of the patient’s swallowing ability when used in conjunction with other information obtained from the clinical swallow evaluation (lagarde, kamalski & van den engel-hoek, 2015; ramsey et al., 2003). indirect laryngoscopy was the only method that was never (0%) used usually or always. as many as 97.3% of participants never and seldom used it. it is evident that the use of this procedure is out of date, as there is very little supporting literature or recent research available on its relevance in the clinical swallow evaluation (ponka & baddar, 2013). the indirect laryngoscopy procedure observes the vocal folds at rest and during phonation and is therefore physically irrelevant as it does not provide information on vocal fold competence during swallowing (ponka & baddar, 2013). it is therefore expected that few speech-language therapists would be trained to perform such a procedure and those who were trained would perhaps not use it. this procedure is also most often performed by the ear, nose and throat specialist. the poor availability and cost of laryngeal mirrors and time constraints may also limit the frequency of its use. refer to methodology with regard to reasoning for including this clinical component. it is also valuable to observe evidence-based practice among participants in this study by the fact that this method is not utilised. the majority of participants (89.4%) usually and always carried out a clinical swallow evaluation before an instrumental evaluation. the remaining 10.6% of participants reported doing this less frequently. the clinical swallow evaluation provides important information regarding the patient’s oral motor and sensory functioning, ability to follow instructions and the nature of the swallow and it also provides a natural setting for eating and drinking. the clinical swallow evaluation is important for first considering the effectiveness of various postural swallowing techniques and adaptive feeding measures in preventing aspiration. their effectiveness is confirmed with an instrumental evaluation (gonzález-fonández et al., 2013b). omission of the clinical swallow evaluation could be due to doctors or radiographers summoning the speech-language therapist to the instrumental evaluation without the knowledge of an initial clinical swallow evaluation or limited human resources, resulting in less time to perform both a clinical swallow evaluation and an instrumental evaluation for every adult with a stroke. although more than 90% of participants displayed good clinical practice by frequently using 41% of clinical components in the clinical swallow evaluation, the remaining 59% of clinical components were used infrequently. despite receiving education, training and policies guiding practice it is understandable that frequency of clinical component utilisation would differ due to: (1) patient-specific requirements and the nature of the clinical swallow evaluation, which is applicable anywhere around the world, (2) the south african context where varying policies are established and (3) where resource availability is limited. each adult with neurogenic dysphagia post-acute stroke presents with varying difficulties and capabilities, thus requiring individual needs and adaptations in the clinical swallow evaluation. it is the speech-language therapist’s duty to use his or her discretion and tailor the clinical swallow evaluation individually to the needs of the adult with stroke and include or exclude clinical components as he or she sees appropriate (american speech-language-hearing association, 2004). the clinical swallow evaluation is thus exploratory in nature and is susceptible to change according to the speech-language therapist’s judgement (saslha, 2011). scope of practice and policies with guidelines for practice set by the hpcsa (2009) and saslha (2011) are vague and incomplete. clinical components that are supported by the literature and should be included in the clinical swallow evaluation are omitted, for example assessing sensation of oral motor structures. similar policies of international standards such as the american speech-language-hearing association (2004) or the speech pathology association of australia ltd (2012) provide in-depth and complete guidelines and scope of practice detailing all clinical components that should be used in the clinical swallow evaluation. speech-language therapists in south africa do not all have access to policies from saslha (2011) and some do not know that the hpcsa’s (2009) guidelines for scope of practice exist. speech-language therapists tend to rely on training from university, continued professional development events and advice from colleagues with regard to what the clinical swallow evaluation should include, instead of adhering to guidelines for practice (modi & ross, 2000). another reason why the clinical swallow evaluation is likely to vary among speech-language therapists is due to the south african context where availability of resources is limited. approximately three-quarters of african countries, including south africa, receive the lowest proportion of government funding for health care (29.5%) compared with high-income countries (42%; george et al., 2009). lowand middle-income countries have a greater lack of resources and poor access to health care compared with high-income countries. given the lower socio-economic status and the massive economic burden due to stroke in south africa there is a shortage of trained and skilled health professionals and a shortage of medical facilities and equipment for stroke (mills et al., 2011). the consistency of use of clinical components by participants high rates of variability in dysphagia practice were found in this study, consistent with the results of previous studies (bateman et al., 2007; carnaby & harenberg, 2013; martino, pron & diamant, 2004; mathers-schmidt & kurlinski, 2003). it was interesting that gag reflex testing was used inconsistently given its poor support in the literature (mccullough et al., 2005; oliveira et al., 2015). it seems that speech-language therapists are not sure whether or not to include this clinical component in the clinical swallow evaluation. social history and background information may not be used consistently due to time constraints where assessing the swallow is more of a priority. it is, however, surprising that current medication use is inconsistent and may be due to the speech-language therapist thinking that is the doctor’s area of expertise or that the cause of the dysphagia is due solely to the stroke. poor training at an undergraduate level and at a post-graduate level may also be a factor. consistency of clinical practice is desirable; however, it is neither rigid nor prescribed (carnaby & harenberg, 2013). it has been known to improve the quality of health care (carnaby & harenberg, 2013), and thus adaptations to consistency of practice are endorsed. a high rate of variability in clinical practice is expected in an economically developing country like south africa where different education and training programmes are established at different universities. singh et al. (2015) discovered that not only were these education and training programmes varied, but four out of six universities provided inadequate theoretical and clinical training. this leaves half of speech-language therapists feeling unprepared and insufficiently trained in adult dysphagia to perform a clinical swallow evaluation without supervision in the working world (singh et al., 2015). new speech-language therapists may omit assessing some clinical components in the clinical swallow evaluation due to nervousness or due to lack of theoretical knowledge, training and experience (singh et al., 2015). ideally, new graduates should practise under supervision of an experienced colleague for about 6 months (saslha, 2011), but often in south africa this is not possible due to the shortage of staff or due to the fact that new graduates are often the only ones placed at a health institution to increase outreach of health services to the public. the consequences of this include unsatisfactory and potentially unsafe patient management and the speech-language therapist avoiding contributing to the burden of limited access to health care. speech-language therapists do not always practise from research-based theory, but rather from experience. experience results in increased levels of confidence and influences practice. clinical competency and expertise, and preference and attitude towards practice also play a role in practice patterns (riquelme, 2015). practice in south africa is based on experience at an undergraduate level and in the working world as well as from the opinions of experienced colleagues, thus not being evidence-based (steele et al., 2007). barriers to continuing education include time constraints, geographical problems, lack of available courses and financial difficulties (steele et al., 2007). there can also be a lack of access to articles and research, poor aptitude in identifying information from articles and reduced perceived value and relevance of information found (nail-chiwetalu & ratner, 2007). canmeds, a framework for medical health professional competency of practice, highlights the importance of life-long learning and continued professional education as well as the translation, distribution and application of learned knowledge (frank, 2005). resources in the clinical swallowing evaluation physical resources more than half of responses indicated poor availability of resources. speech-language therapists are inclined to make alternative arrangements in evaluation contexts when resources are limited. for example, to adapt food and liquid consistencies without modifying agents by using the south african food amasi (fermented milk). this is a liquid with a thicker consistency than milk and is relatively cheap. speech-language therapists are forced to think ‘out of the box’ and to become practical with little or no resources in the clinical swallow evaluation. otherwise, infrequent use of resources was logical, and it seems the speech-language therapist uses physical resources sparingly. the amount of resources used was approximately the same in both public and private health sectors: 42.1% and 57.9%, respectively. however, about 75% of responses mentioning unavailability of physical resources came from participants in the public health sector. fifty per cent of the participants in the private health sector did not use resources due to unavailability, while the remaining 50% concerned poor training, time limits, patient-specific needs and gaps in protocol. the allocation of financial resources in the public health sector is inadequately managed. due to poor administrative and managerial capacities and infrastructure the availability of physical resources is often limited (mayosi et al., 2012). the private health sector receives financial contributions nine times the amount the public health sector receives; hence, there is more money appropriately allocated towards physical resources (harris et al., 2011). the public–private divide in health sectors in south africa shows an inequitable and inefficient distribution of resources. medical resources blackwell and littlejohns (2010) also detected poor usage of medical resources in the clinical swallow evaluation due to the absence thereof. it was mentioned that there is a lack of financial resources specifically for equipment, such as stethoscopes for performing cervical auscultation procedures and oximeters for conducting pulse oximetry procedures. there was a desire from participants to receive further training with the use of such medical resources; however, there may be a lack of funding or support for such training preand post-graduation. in the light of a context such as south africa, it is comforting to see that more than 90% of participants in the study had access to consumable resources usually and always. non-medical resources: infrequent use of food and liquid modifying agents could be due to limited availability and patient-specific needs. adults with stroke may not require thickeners and often do not like the taste of such products. the fact that their diet is modified at all is alone the cause for a lowered quality of life (swan, speyer, heijnen, wagg & cordier, 2015). infrequent use of adapted eating utensils may have been due to the fact that regular eating utensils are favoured over adaptive feeding tools as they are less expensive and do not stand out in a social eating situation. some adults with neurogenic dysphagia do not want to be seen in social situations eating with adapted eating utensils. some participants mentioned trying to aim for typical eating utensils in therapy. human resources: multidisciplinary teamwork is key for interprofessional communication and identifying common patient goals (trapl et al., 2007). there is little regard for other health professionals on the team. doctors and nurses have been reported to have a poor awareness and regard for speech-language therapists and their role in neurogenic dysphagia (albini, soares, wolf & goncalves, 2013). one note of concern was that staff changes often caused problems in terms of continuing recommended feeding practices and diets. speech-language therapists need to document their recommendations extensively and clearly after a clinical swallow evaluation so that other speech-language therapists and other health professionals can continue to engage in correct and safe feeding practices. there is a drastic shortage of health professionals in south africa compared with high-income countries, especially in the public health sector and in rural areas (george et al., 2009; mills et al., 2011). for every patient seen by a specialist in the private health sector 23 are seen by a specialist in the public health sector (george et al., 2009). reasons for such human resource shortages include migration of health professionals, the ageing of the nurse population and the increasing burden of disease and illness (george et al., 2009). the extension of the national antiretroviral treatment programme for hiv and aids has also drawn nurses away from other health services (george et al., 2009). it is evident that access to resources is most often a challenge in a context like south africa. reasons for poor resource use in the clinical swallow evaluation are indicative of south africa’s poor resource availability and insufficient training. the speech-language therapist therefore uses certain resources less frequently and needs to adapt the clinical swallow evaluation to suit the limited availability of resources and the needs of the adult with acute stroke. limitations as clinical practice represents one’s ability to manage a patient optimally, participants may have been more likely to report higher frequencies of clinical component use due to these relatively sensitive questions. this observational error could have occurred due to knowledge of scope of practice and service expectations and to avoid embarrassment (groves, 2004). the sample selection methods may have been biased due to using only one organisation to recruit participants. this may have obtained an unrepresentative sample of the population, as participants did not have an equal chance of being selected for the study (leedy & ormrod, 2013). a bigger sample size could have been obtained; however, due to the nature of electronic surveys, response rates are often low (cook, heath & thompson, 2000). generalisation of results must be considered with caution and observations can only be made with regard to the participants in this study. the qualitative data component was subject to theme identification by the researcher only, of which the reliability has been known to be poor. it is however widely accepted for researchers to review their own data, especially as it is not a mixed method study (carnaby & harenberg, 2013; mathers-schmidt & kurlinski, 2003). validity and reliability the researcher conducted a narrative review of the literature to adapt the already existing questionnaire from other validated studies (green et al., 2006). the reliability of the narrative review was increased by having external reviewers blindly review databases (leedy & ormrod, 2013). one hundred per cent inter-rater agreement was reached for any disparities between reviewers. construct validity was improved by keeping search terms consistent among reviewers and by selecting reviewers who were also speech-language therapists thus having an adequate understanding of the research topic and search terms (leedy & ormrod, 2013). a pilot study was conducted to identify any weaknesses in the questionnaire’s content and layout and with regard to its applicability in the south african context (leedy & ormrod, 2013). this improved internal consistency reliability and construct validity (leedy & ormrod, 2013). by having south african participants take part in the pilot study the terminology could be verified as appropriate or not and contextual factors influencing questions or answers could be commented on. developing a questionnaire that was similar to, motivated from and adapted from other, recent and validated studies increased the content validity and criterion-related validity (leedy & ormrod, 2013). reliability was also improved by eliminating interview bias by administering the questionnaire by only electronic means (leedy & ormrod, 2013). the questionnaire was worded and presented clearly and unambiguously, and without misleading tendencies or persuasion (babbie & mouton, 2001) to ensure a greater likelihood of truthful and relevant answers. ethical considerations ethical permission from the biomedical research ethics committee at the university of kwazulu-natal was granted in april 2015. participation in the study was voluntary and the participant could withdraw from the study at any time. consent was indicated by their signing the consent form, and all responses were kept confidential and anonymous (leedy & ormrod, 2013). participants were not subjected to any physical, psychological or disclosure dangers and were not obliged to participate. participants benefitted only by receiving feedback of the study’s results. copies of the completed questionnaires were kept confidential and will be stored electronically at the university of kwazulu-natal for up to 5 years post-completion of the study and thereafter digitally destroyed with two staff members who will bear witness to the procedure. arbitrary numbers were allocated to each completed questionnaire and raw data responses were shared with only the statistician (leedy & ormrod, 2013). conclusion varying adherence to official policies and fluctuating consistency of clinical practice among speech-language therapists in this study indicate a need for more uniformity of education and training curricula at south african universities as well as for more supervision after graduation. it is also apparent that continued professional development is important for updated knowledge and practice. a means by which speech-language therapists can access recent literature is vital in order to maintain knowledge and engage in evidence-based practice. policies set out to guide clinical practice need perhaps to be more detailed, more easily available and more recognised. a suitable clinical swallowing evaluation policy specifically for a resource-limited context is also needed, and more human, financial and physical resources are also essential. ultimately, the adult with neurogenic dysphagia post-acute stroke may not benefit fully from such a context and from inconsistent clinical practice in south africa. more research is needed with regard to the frequency of clinical component use, the consistency of its use and reasons why it is not used in the clinical swallow evaluation of adults with neurogenic dysphagia. reviews of what current curricula entail at south african universities are needed to determine how many hours of theoretical and practical training are completed and which clinical components are being taught. current policies and guidelines also need to be reviewed to address uniformity and comprehensiveness among them. a suitable clinical swallowing evaluation policy for a resource-limited context is also needed. more research is needed potentially to motivate for an improved curriculum at an educational level through which a more consistent clinical swallow evaluation protocol with a constant set of clinical components is taught at universities across south africa. this is in the hope that the use and consistency of clinical components by speech-language therapists in the clinical swallowing evaluation of adults with neurogenic dysphagia post-acute stroke will improve. acknowledgements competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships which may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions m.p. was the supervisor of the study and reviewed and co-authored this article. the supervisor guided the data collection and oversaw the overall relevance and flow of the article. m.b.a. conducted the research and compiled the article. references albini, r.m.n., soares, v.m.n., wolf, a.e., & goncalves, c.g.d.o. (2013). knowledge of nursing professionals about the care to dysphagic patients in intensive care units. revista cefac, 15(6), 1512–1524. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s1516-18462013005000047 american speech-language-hearing association. (2004). preferred practice patterns for the profession of speech-language pathology. [preferred practice patterns]. retrieved june 29, 2014, from http://www.asha.org/policya american speech-language-hearing association. (2005). evidence-based practice in communication disorders. [position statement]. retrieved from http://www.asha.org/policy/ps2005-00221.htm babbie, e., & mouton, j. (2001). the practice of social research: south african edition. cape town, south africa: oxford university press. balzer, k.m. (2000). drug-induced dysphagia. international journal of ms care, 2(1), 6–7. http://dx.doi.org/10.7224/1537-2073-2.1.40 bateman, c., leslie, p., & drinnan, m.j. (2007). adult dysphagia assessment in the uk and ireland: are slts assessing the same factors? dysphagia, 22, 174–186. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00455-006-9070-3 blackwell, z., & littlejohns, p. (2010). a review of the management of dysphagia: a south african perspective. american association of neuroscience nurses, 42(2), 61–70. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/jnn.0b013e3181ce5c86 bless, c., higson-smith, c., & kagee, a. (2006). fundamentals of social research methods: an african perspective. (4th edn.). cape town, south africa: juta & company ltd. carnaby, g.d., & harenberg, l. (2013). what is ‘usual care’ in dysphagia rehabilitation: a survey of usa dysphagia practice patterns. dysphagia, 28, 567–574. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00455-013-9467-8 chong, m.s., lieu, p.k., sitoh, y.y., meng, y.y., & leow, l.p. (2003). bedside clinical methods useful as screening test for aspiration in elderly patients with recent and previous strokes. annals of the academy of medicine, singapore, 32(6), 790–794. pmid 14716948. retrieved from http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/14716948 cook, c., heath, f., & thompson, r.l. (2000). a meta-analysis on response rates in webor internet-based surveys. educational and psychological measurement, 60(6), 821–836. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/00131640021970934 davis, d.a., & taylor-vaisey, a. (1997). a systematic review of theoretic concepts, practical experience and research evidence in the adoption of clinical practice guidelines. canadian medical association journal, 157, 408–416. retrieved from http://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/pmc1227916/pdf/cmaj_157_4_408.pdf department of health. (2011). south africa: health profile. world health organisation. retrieved december 26, 2015, from http://www.health.gov.za/index.php/2014-03-17-09-09-38/2014-03-17-12-57-15 frank, j.r. (ed.). (2005). the canmeds 2005 physician competency framework. better standards. better physicians. better care. ottawa: the royal college of physicians and surgeons of canada. retrieved from http://www.ub.edu/medicina_unitateducaciomedica/documentos/canmeds.pdf george, g., quinlan, t., & reardon, c. (2009). human resources for health: a gaps and needs analysis of hrh in south africa. retrieved from http://www.heard.org.za/downloads/human-resources-for-health--a-needs-and-gaps-analysis-of-hrh-in-south-africa.pdf gonzález-fonández, m., huckabee, m., doeltgen, s.h., inamoto, y., kagaya, h., & saitoh, e. (2013a). dysphagia rehabilitation: similarities and differences in three areas of the world. current physical medicine rehabilitation and reports, 1, 296–306. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s40141-013-0035-9 gonzález-fonández, m., ottenstein, l., atanelov, l., & christian, a.b. (2013b). dysphagia after stroke: an overview. current physical medical rehabilitation reports, 1, 187–196. green, b.n., johnson, c.d., & adams, a. (2006). writing narrative literature reviews for peer-reviewed journals: secrets of the trade. journal of chiropractic medicine, 5, 101–117. groves, r.m. (2004). survey errors and survey costs. hoboken, nj: john wiley & sons, inc. guyomard, v., fulcher, r.a., redmayne, o., metcalf, a.k., potter, j.f., & myint, p.k. (2009). effect of dysphasia and dysphagia on inpatient mortality and hospital length of stay: a database study. journal of american geriatrics society, 57, 2101–2106. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1532-5415.2009.02526.x harris, b., goudge, j., ataguba, j.e., mcintyre, d., nxumalo, n., jikwana, s., et al. (2011). inequities in access to health care in south africa. journal of public health policy, 32, s102–s123. http://dx.doi.org/10.1057/jphp.2011.35 health professions council of south africa (hpcsa). (2009). regulations defining the scope of the profession of speech-language therapy. retrieved december 2, 2015, from http://www.medmall.co.za/medmall/2006/services/sashla/scope_of_profession_speech_final_21_nov%202009.pdf heinemann, a.w., roth, e.j., rychlik, k., pe, k., king, c., & clumpner, j. (2003). the impact of stroke practice guidelines on knowledge and practice patterns of acute care health professionals. journal of evaluation in clinical practice, 9(2), 203–212. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2753.2003.00380.x lagarde, m.l.j., kamalski, d.m.a., & van den engel-hoek, l. (2015). the reliability and validity of cervical auscultation in the diagnosis of dysphagia: a systematic review. clinical rehabilitation, 30(2), 199–207. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0269215515576779 leedy, p.d., & ormrod, j.e. (2013). practical research: planning and design. (10th edn.). boston, ma: pearson education, inc. mander, m., ntuli, l., diederichs, n., & mavundla, k. (2007). economics of the traditional medicine trade in south africa: health care delivery. south african health review, 189–196. retrieved from http://reference.sabinet.co.za/webx/access/electronic_journals/healthr/healthr_2007_a16.pdf maredza, m., bertram, m.y., & tollman, s.m. (2015). disease burden of stroke in rural south africa: an estimate of incidence, mortality and disability adjusted life years. biomed central neurology, 15(54), 2–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/s12883-015-0311-7 martino, r., foley, n., bhogal, s., diamant, n., speechley, m., & teasell, r. (2005). dysphagia after stroke: incidence, diagnosis, and pulmonary complications. stroke, 36, 2756–2763. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.str.0000190056.76543.eb martino, r., pron, g., & diamant, n.e. (2004). oropharyngeal dysphagia: surveying practice patterns of the speech-language pathologist. dysphagia, 19, 165–176. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00455-004-0004-7 mathers-schmidt, b.a., & kurlinski, m. (2003). dysphagia evaluation practices: inconsistencies in clinical assessment and instrumental examination decision-making. dysphagia, 18, 114–125. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00455-002-0094-z mayosi, b.m., lawn, j.e., van niekerk, a., bradshaw, d., karim, s.s.a., coovadia, h.m., et al. (2012). health in south africa: changes and challenges since 2009. the lancet, 308(9858), 2029–2043. http://dx.doi.org/10/1016/s0140-6736(12)61814-5 mccullough, g.h., rosenbek, j.c., wertz, r.t., mccoy, s., mann, g., & mccullough, k. (2005). utility of clinical swallowing examination measures for detecting aspiration post-stroke. journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 48, 1280–1293. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2005/089) mills, e.j., kanters, s., hagopian, a., bansback, n., nachega, j., alberton, m., et al. (2011). the financial cost of doctors emigrating from sub-saharan africa: human capital analysis. british medical journal, 343, 1–13. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/bmj.d7031 modi, n., & ross, e. (2000). the current practices, training and concerns of a group of hospital-based speech-language pathologists working in the area of dysphagia. the south african journal of communication disorders, 47, 3–14. pmid 11455821 nail-chiwetalu, b., & ratner, n.b. (2007). an assessment of the information-seeking abilities and needs of practicing speech-language pathologists. journal of the medical library association, 95(2), 182–188, e56–e57. national development plan. (2012). executive summary-national development plan 2030: our future: make it work. retrieved december 26, 2015, from http://www.gov.za/sites/www.gov.za/files/executive%20summary-ndp%202030%20-%20our%20future%20-%20make%20it%20work.pdf oliveira, a.r., costa, a.g., morais, h.c.c., cavalcante, t.f., lopes, m.v., & araujo, t.l. (2015). clinical factors predicting risk for aspiration and respiratory aspiration among patients with stroke. revista latino-americana de enfermagem, 23(2), 216–224. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/0104-1169.0197.2545 pettigrew, c.m., & o’toole, c. (2007). dysphagia evaluation practices in speech and language therapists in ireland: clinical assessment and instrumental examination decision-making. dysphagia, 22, 235–244. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00455-007-9079-2 pillay, m., kathard, h., & samuel, m.a. (1997). a curriculum of practice: a conceptual framework for speech-language therapy and audiology practice with a black-african first language clientele. the south african journal of communication disorders, 44, 109–117. retrieved from http://europepmc.org/abstract/med/9819973 ponka, d., & baddar, f. (2013). top 10 forgotten diagnostic procedures: indirect laryngoscopy. canadian family physician, 29(11), 1201. retrieved from http://www.cfp.ca/content/59/11/12%2001 ramsey, d.j.c., smithard, d.g., & kalra, l. (2003). early assessments of dysphagia and aspiration risk in acute stroke patients. journal of the american heart association: stroke, 34, 1252–1257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.str.0000066309.06490.b8 riquelme, l.f. (2007). the role of cultural competence in providing services to persons with dysphagia. topics in geriatric rehabilitation, 23(3), 228–239. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/01.tgr.0000284767.38172.af riquelme, l.f. (2013). cultural competence for everyone: a shift in perspectives. perspectives in gerontology, 18(2), 42–49. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/gero18.2.42 riquelme, l.f. (2015). clinical swallow examination (cse): can we talk? perspectives on swallowing and swallowing disorders (dysphagia), 24, 34–39. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/sasd24.1.34 rogers, b., & arvedson, j. (2005). assessment of infant oral sensorimotor and swallowing function. mental retardation and developmental disabilities, 11, 74–82. sajjad, a., chowdhury, r., felix, j.f, ikram, m.a., mendis, s., tiemeier, h., et al. (2013). a systematic evaluation of stroke surveillance studies in lowand middle-income countries. neurology, 80(7), 677–684. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/wnl.0b013e318281cc6e schiele, j.t., penner, h., schneider, h., quinzler, r., reich, g., wezler, n., et al. (2015). swallowing tablets and capsules increases the risk of penetration and aspiration in the patients with stroke-induced dysphagia. dysphagia, 30(5), 571–582. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00455-015-9639-9 schroeder, m.f., daniels, s.k., mcclain, m., corey, d.m., & foundas, a.l. (2006). clinical cognitive predictors of swallowing recovery in stroke. journal of rehabilitation research and development, 43(3), 301–310. http://dx.doi.org/10.1682/jrrd.2004.12.01547040 singh, s., booth, a., choto, f., gotlieb, j., robertson, r., morris, g., et al. (2015). new graduates’ perceptions of preparedness to provide speech-language therapy services in general and dysphagia services in particular. south african journal of communication disorders, 62(1), 1–8. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v62i1.110 smith hammond, c.a., & goldstein, l.b. (2006). cough and aspiration on food and liquids due to oral-pharyngeal dysphagia: accp evidence-based clinical practice guidelines. chest, 129, 154s–168s. http://dx.doi.org/10.1378/chest.129.1_suppl.154s the south african speech-language-hearing association (saslha). (2011). guidelines: dysphagia in adults. retrieved from http://www.mm3admin.co.za/documents/docmanager/55e836d5-3332-4452-bb05-9f12be8da9d8/00012511.pdf the speech pathology association of australia ltd. (2012). dysphagia clinical guidelines. retrieved from http://www.speechpathologyaustralia.org.au/library/clinical-_guidelines/final_15062012_dysphagia_clinical_guidelines.pdf statistics south africa. (2014). mortality and causes of death in south africa, 2013: findings from death notification. pretoria: statistics south africa. retrieved from http://www.statssa.gov.za/publications/p03093/p030932013.pdf steele, c.m., allen, c., barker, j., buen, p., french, r., fedorak, a., et al. (2007). dysphagia service delivery by speech-language pathologists in canada: results of a national survey. canadian journal of speech-language pathology and audiology, 31(4), 166–177. retrieved from http://cjslpa.ca/files/2007_cjslpa_vol_31/no_04_161-200/steele_cjslpa_2007.pdf straus, s.e., tetroe, j., & graham, i. (2009). defining knowledge translation. canadian medical association, 181, 3–4. http://dx.doi.org/10.1503/cmaj.081229 swan, k., speyer, r., heijnen, b.j., wagg, b., & cordier, r. (2015). living with oropharyngeal dysphagia: effects of bolus modification on health-related quality of life – a systematic review. quality of life research, 10(24), 2447–2456. trapl, m., enderle, p., nowotny, m., teuschl, y., matz, k., dachenhausen, a., et al. (2007). dysphagia bedside screening for acute stroke patients: the gugging swallowing screen. stroke, 38, 2948–2952. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/strokeaha.107.483933 van peppen, r.p.s., hendriks, h.j.m., meeteren, n.l.u., helders, p.j.m., & kwakkel, g. (2007). the development of a clinical practice stroke guidelines for physiotherapists in the netherlands: a systematic review of available evidence. disability and rehabilitation, 29(10), 767–783. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/09638280600919764 seema panday, harsha kathard, mershen pillay, cyril govender the homogeneity of audibility and prosody of zulu words for speech reception threshold (srt) testing *seema panday, #harsha kathard, #mershen pillay, *cyril govender *university of kwazulu-natal #university of cape town abstract t h e a im o f t h i s i n v e s t i g a t i o n w a s t o d e t e r m i n e w h i c h o f 5 8 p r e s e l e c t e d z u l u w o r d s d e v e l o p e d by p a n d a y e t a l . ( 2 0 0 7 ) c o u l d b e u s e d f o r s p e e c h r e c e p t i o n t h r e s h o l d ( s r t ) t e s t i n g . t o r e a l i z e t h i s a i m t h e h o m o g e n e i t y o f a u d i b i l i t y o f 5 8 b i s y l l a b i c z u l u l o w t o n e v e r b s w a s m e a s u r e d , f o l l o w e d by an a n a l y s i s o f t h e p r o s o d i c f e a t u r e s o f t h e s e l e c t e d w o r d s . t h e w o r d s w e r e d i g i t a l l y r e c o r d e d b y a z u l u f i r s t l a n g u a g e m a l e s p e a k e r a n d p r e s e n t e d a t 6 i n t e n s i t y l e v e l s t o 3 0 z u l u f i r s t l a n g u a g e s p e a k e r s ( 1 8 2 5 y e a r s , m e a n a g e o f 2 1 . 5 y e a r s ) , w h o s e h e a r i n g w a s n o r m a l . h o m o g e n e i t y o f a u ­ d i b i l i t y w a s d e t e r m i n e d b y e m p l o y i n g l o g i s t i c r e g r e s s i o n a n a l y s i s . t w e n t y e i g h t w o r d s m e t t h e c r i ­ t e r i o n o f h o m o g e n e i t y o f a u d i b i l i t y . t h i s w a s e v i d e n c e d by a m e a n s l o p e o f 5 0 % a t 5 . 9 8 % / d b . t h e p r o s o d i c f e a t u r e s o f t h e t w e n t y e i g h t w o r d s w e r e f u r t h e r a n a l y z e d u s i n g a c o m p u t e r i z e d s p e e c h l a b o r a t o r y s y s t e m . t h e f i n d i n g s c o n f i r m e d t h a t t h e p i t c h c o n t o u r s o f t h e w o r d s f o l l o w e d t h e p r o ­ s o d i c p a t t e r n a p p a r e n t w i t h i n z u l u l i n g u i s t i c s t r u c t u r e . e i g h t y n i n e p e r c e n t o f t h e z u l u v e r b s w e r e f o u n d t o h a v e a d i f f e r e n c e i n t h e p i t c h p a t t e r n b e t w e e n t h e t w o s y l l a b l e s i . e . t h e f i r s t s y l l a b l e w a s l o w i n p i t c h , w h i l e t h e s e c o n d s y l l a b l e w a s h i g h i n p i t c h . i t e m e r g e d t h a t t h e t w e n t y e i g h t w o r d s c o u l d b e u s e d f o r e s t a b l i s h i n g s r t w i t h i n a n o r m a l h e a r i n g z u l u s p e a k i n g p o p u l a t i o n . f u r ­ t h e r r e s e a r c h w i t h i n c l i n i c a l p o p u l a t i o n s i s r e c o m m e n d e d . k e y w o r d s s p e e c h r e c e p t i o n t h r e s h o l d , z u l u , z u l u f i r s t l a n g u a g e s p e a k e r s , h o m o g e n e i t y o f a u d i ­ b i l i t y , a c o u s t i c a n a l y s i s , p r o s o d i c f e a t u r e s the selection of words for srt testing in languages other than english has recently received much attention in the literature. this follows nissen, harris, jennings, eggett & buck’s (2005) assertion that it is essential that srt testing be conducted in the language of the listener. given this imperative and to attain valid and reliable srt results, research has been conducted in many languages such as arabic (ashoor & proshazka, 1985); tiwi (plant, 1990); walpiri (plant, 1990); brazilian portuguese (harris, goffi, pedalini, gygi & merrill, 2001); korean (harris et al., 2003); polish (harris et al., 2004); mandarin (nissen et al., 2005) and taiwanese mandarin (nissen, harris & dukes, 2008). however, srt word lists in african languages such as zulu have received very little or no attention although zulu is spoken by almost 8.5 million people in south africa (grimes, 1992). there­ fore, it is necessary that a linguistically-matched srt word list be formulated according to specific criteria to enable the appropriate assessment of hearing of the many zulu speaking south africans, the majority of whom reside in the province of kwazulu-natal (kzn). panday, kathard, pillay & govender (2007) have described the developmental process of adapting criteria for srt word lists by considering zulu linguistic structure. the adaptation of the origi­ nal srt criteria to suit the linguistic structure of the language, for 60 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, which the list is being developed, has received strong support in international literature. evidence of the development of srt word lists in other mother tongue languages is seen in arabic word lists (ashoor & proshazka, 1985); walpiri & tiwi (plant, 1990); manda­ rin (nissen et al. 2005) & taiwanese mandarin (nissen et al. 2008). panday et al. (2007) have reported on the identification and selection of fifty eight zulu words that met the following lin­ guistic criteria i.e. the words were bisyllabic, phonetically dissimi­ lar, familiar, low tone verb imperatives. these criteria were adapted from the original srt criteria as specified by hudgins, hawkins, karlin & stevens (1947) i.e. the words had to lie famil­ iar, bisyllabic, equally stressed words (spondees) that were pho­ netically dissimilar, and which had to be homogeneous for audi­ bility. the focus of this paper is to describe which of the fifty eight preselected zulu words met the criterion of homogeneity of audibility. in addition, a description of the prosodic features of the most homogenous words selected was made, so that the words finally selected from this process could be used for srt testing within a selected zulu first language speaking population. contact: seema panday discipline of audiology university of kwazulu -n atal westville campus durban south africa email: pandavse@ukzn.ac.za tel: 031-2607623 ' vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) mailto:pandavse@ukzn.ac.za the homogeneity of audibility and prosody homogeneity of audibility has been described as the ease at which the words are understood when spoken at a constant level of intensity (silman & silverman, 1991). it is regarded as one of the most important criteria for the selection of srt test materials (nissen et al., 2005). homogeneity of audibility of words in a list can be achieved in two ways, i.e. by selecting only those words that reach the listener's ear at the same intensity or by recording the individual words in such a way that they all tend to be heard at the same level of production (beattie, svihohee & edgerton, 1975). schill (1985) explained that in order to meet the criterion of homogeneity of audibility, the percentage of words correctly recognized must increase rapidly with a relatively small increase in intensity. this is usually illustrated through the use of the per formance-intensity curve (brandy, 2002). the performance inten­ sity curve is the more recent term used for the psychometric func­ tion which describes the relationship between some measure of performance and a stimulus (kruger & kruger, 1997). the performance intensity curve serves to illustrate how well the speech sample (words in the list) is correctly identified as a function of various intensity levels. the steepness of the curve determines the precision with which the threshold can be ob­ tained (hudgins, hawkins, karlin & stevens, 1947). nissen et al. (2005) added that the steepness or slope of the performance intensity curve is an important factor to consider in selecting words for srt testing. hence, these aspects were considered when determining which of 58 zulu words preselected by panday et al. (2007) met the criterion of homogeneity of audibility. however, obtaining ideal steep performance intensity curves together with homogeneity of audibility has posed serious chal­ lenges over the years. the literature seems to indicate that the l number of words selected is reduced when the criterion for ho-i mogeneity of audibility is applied. this was seen when the original srt word list in english was (hirsh, silverman, reynolds further reduced to 22 words reduced from 84 words to 36 words eldert & benson, 1952) and was (bowling & elpern, 1961). i bowling and elpern’s (19,61) findings were supported by re­ search conducted by curry'and .cox (1966) who found similar results. the range of intelligibility between the words in the curry and cox (1966) study for normal hearers was 8%/db compared to 10%/db in the bowling and elpern (1961) study. the homoge­ neity of audibility of the words suggested in the above studies was also challenged by beattie et al. (1975). these authors ques­ tioned whether the word differences noted in the bowling and elpern (1961) study were due either to .inconsistencies in the recording process or due to the selection of the words. later, young et al. (1982) explained these discrepancies by presenting a more stringent methodological framework for deter­ mining how words for srt testing in english met the criterion of homogeneity of audibility. young et al. (1982) postulated that of zulu words for speech reception threshold testing previous studies were confounded by the learning effect that occurred within the word lists. bowling and elpern (1961) and beattie et al. (1975) tested the same randomized words at sev­ eral intensity levels using a 2db step increase in the intensity. young et al. (1982) regarded this as a confounding factor as the participants were possibly performing better because of having learnt the words in the list rather than using hearing acuity to determine loudness or clarity of the words. more recently, however, nissen et al. (2005) utilized a detailed mathematical model to determine which of 138 mandarin words met the criterion of homogeneity of audibility. these authors pro­ vided a motivation for the use of the logistic regression model for the measurement of homogeneity of audibility. the logistic re­ gression model according to nissen et al. (2005) provided a de­ scription of the slope at 50% of the performance intensity curves of each word, the estimated threshold of intensity at which each word was heard and individual performance intensity curves for the words. this allows the researcher to rapidly identify those words that are being heard easily compared to the words that were not (nissen et al., 2005). the advantage of using this model is that the rate of change of intelligibility is accurate and available instantly. in addition, nissen et al. (2005) described favourable results with regard to the slope at 50% for mandarin words. there was an almost equivalent performance with that of the english words. the 11.3 percent per decibel (%/db) obtained in the nissen et al. (2005) study was perhaps due to a digital adjustment made to the intensity of each of the words, so that the 50% of each word selected was equal to the mean pure tone average of the participants in the study. this adjustment controlled for variability of the performance intensity curves and thereby improved the overall homogeneity of audibility of the words. clearly, the measurement of homogeneity of audibility of the words for srt testing is not a simple issue. homogeneity of audi­ bility of the words and the performance intensity function of each word are not only influenced by methodological and procedural variables as described earlier, but can also be influenced by sev­ eral other factors that have been recently discussed in the litera­ ture. some of these factors include, the type of stimuli used (kruger & kruger, 1997), the number of syllables in the words (chetty, 1990), the recording (nissen et al., 2005) and calibration of equipment (lyregaard, 1997), the choice of the speaker used for the recording of the words and most importantly the structure of language. it is important that test material consists of items familiar to the listener. the items must represent the real world of the listener (chermack & musiek, 1997). this contention appears relevant to the measurement of homogeneity of the words in zulu. therefore, apart from the acoustic-phonetic characteristics of the words, the the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 61 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) seema panday, harsha kathard, mershen pillay, cyril govender lexical processes, such as the semantic, syntactic and content of the words can also influence the way words are heard. conse­ quently, a "steeper performance intensity curve results when the task is simple, the stimuli are familiar, and most homogene­ ous" (kruger & kruger, 1997, p. 234). therefore, within the pre­ sent study the 58 preselected zulu words were items that were most familiar in the zulu language. this is also in keeping with the suggestions made by borg, wilson & samuelsson (1998) who highlighted the importance of selecting familiar items for srt testing. panday et al. (2007) provide a detailed explanation of how the 58 preselected zulu words met the criterion of familiar­ ity. another factor that influences the homogeneity of audibility of the words is the number of syllables in the word. bisyllabic words were traditionally used for srt testing. however, the study con­ ducted by nissen et al. (2005) obtained steep performance inten­ sity curves with trisyllabic words. one could argue that the in­ crease in the number of acoustic cues in the trisyllabic words can affect the steepness of the curve because the increase in the acoustic cues available to the listener allows the word to be eas­ ily identified at lower intensities. therefore, measurement of ho­ mogeneity of audibility in the present study could be influenced by the number of syllables in the zulu words. however, given that there is no reference point for what constitutes an acceptable number of acoustic cues, it would be inappropriate to perform cross-linguistic comparisons between bisyllabic words versus trisyllabic words without a fuller semantic-syntactic linguistic analysis. a more critical factor influencing the validity of measure­ ment of homogeneity of audibility, however, is whether the words are presented in recorded versus monitored live voice testing (nissen etal., 2005). recorded and monitored live voice testing methods are both recommended for clinical use (american speech-language hear­ ing association [asha], 1988; brandy, 2002; roeser, valente & horsford-dunn 2000; silman & silverman, 1991; stach, 1998). however, in terms of measuring homogeneity of audibility of the words, it is advised that recorded materials be used (nissen et al., 2005). the use of recorded materials standardises the com­ position and presentation of the test words. furthermore, the recorded method allows for uniform presentation of the test words (asha, 1988). therefore, recorded material was chosen in the present investigation to ensure favourable performance in­ tensity curves. moreover, researchers should be cognizant of the choice of speaker used in the recording of the stimuli. the stimuli must not only be presented in the language of the listener, but the speaker presenting the stimuli must be of a similar linguistic and dialecti­ cal background as the listener (nissen et al., 2005). in light of this suggestion, the speaker in the present investigation was a male zulu first language speaker from kzn. a male speaker was considered, as recent literature has indicated no clear gender differences influencing the homogeneity of audibility (cambron, wilson & shanks, 1991; nissen etal., 2005). thus, the results of homogeneity of audibility may or may not be influenced by the factors discussed above. however, the unique structure of the zulu language may impact on the outcome of homogeneity of audibility of 58 preselected zulu words and de­ serves some discussion and investigation. the consonant system in zulu is considered more complex than that of english. apart from the known stops, fricatives, and ap proximants, zulu also has three prominent click sounds (doke, 1930). the click sounds are orthographically transcribed as /c/ (dental click); /q/ (palato-alveolar click); and the /x/ (lateral click) (doke, 1930). these click sounds appear frequently in the vo­ cabulary of zulu. this has certain implications for the measure­ ments of homogeneity of audibility of the words, i.e. inclusion of click phonemes in the word list may influence the variability in the performance intensity curves of the words that have click sounds. click sounds have a higher spectral energy and this may result in improved audibility of the words that contain clicks. therefore, the structural variation in the language and the fact that zulu is also a tonal language (rycroft & ncgobo, 1979) could influence the measurement of homogeneity of audibility and pose a possi­ ble challenge when determining those words that are most ho­ mogenous. in addition, the role of tone has three distinct functions in zulu, i.e. semantic (affecting the meaning of words), grammatical, and emotional (cope, 1982). these tonal variations may influence the meaning of the stimuli, thus having further implications for speech understanding and identification. tonal variations are lexically significant in zulu (rycroft & ncgobo, 1979), implying that changes in the pitch of the word may correspond with changes in the meaning of the word. hence, the introduction of acoustic analysis via pitch extraction and en­ ergy values is suggested in addition to evaluating the word for i homogeneity of audibility. the inclusion of acoustic analysis i would confirm the tonal patterns of the verbs in zulu. inherent in an acoustic analysis of words is the description of the prosodic features of a language. in considering the prosodic features of zulu, we find that they are different from those of english. there­ fore, an evaluation of the prosodic features of a language via appropriate acoustic analysis serves to improve the validity of the zulu words selected for srt testing. prosodic features of speech usually include length, accent, stress, tone and intonation (fox, 2000). furthermore, intonation refers to the patterns relating to the rise and fall, and the stress in a language (kent & read, 1992).-in considering zulu, we find that it is a “non-stress” language (rycroft & ngcobo, 1979) e.g. ■ bisyllabic words such as “ham ba" meaning ‘go’ in english, do not 62 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the h om ogeneity of audib ility and prosody of zulu w ords for speech reception threshold testing have equally stressed syllables. stress is not used to indicate emphasis nor is it used to differentiate words or syllables as seen in english (cope, 1982). instead, the prominence on syllables is often used to describe emphasis. this can be seen e.g. when low tone bisyllabic verbs are used in srt testing, the lengthening of the vowel on the first syllable results in the second syllable gain­ ing prominence. similarly, plant (1990) reported that the prominence or stress of the trisyllables in tiwi was on the penultimate syllable of the tiwi words. plant (1990) therefore, suggested an acoustic analy­ sis of the words to confirm the prosodic patterns of the tiwi words before the words were considered for inclusion in srt testing. thus, it is strongly suggested that the prosodic features of the selected zulu words also be analyzed acoustically. an acoustic analysis via pitch extraction and energy values of the vowels would serve to confirm the tonal patterns of the verbs selected i.e. to examine whether they are low tone vs high tone verbs. the words selected for srt testing, therefore, would be homogenous in terms of the prosodic features of zulu as well as for audibility. the importance of acoustic analysis in srt word list develop­ ment, however, is lacking in the literature, but the above motiva­ tion together with the understanding of basic acoustics serves as a basis for the inclusion of acoustic analysis in the present inves­ tigation. furthermore, brandy (2002) indicates that the acoustic analysis of speech is important, as speech sound perception is dependant on both acoustic -phonetic factors and the higher order linguistic processing. contributions to the field with regard to selecting words for srt testing in languages other than english are acknowledged. how­ ever, factors influencing the outcome of homogeneity of audibility together with the uniqueness of the zulu language, suggest a need to determine which of the zulu words proposed by panday et al. (2007) meet the criterion of homogeneity of audibility and to describe the prosodic features of the most homogenous words. thus, the research question for this investigation was: which of the 58 preselected zulu words by panday et al. (2007) could be used for srt testing within a zulu speaking population in kzn? method aim the aim of the investigation was to determine which of the 58 preselected zulu words could be used for srt testing within a zulu speaking population in kzn. the following objectives were generated to achieve the above aim. objectives 1. to determine which of the 58 preselected zulu words met the criterion of homogeneity of audibility. 2. to describe the prosodic features, specifically pitch and en­ ergy contours, of the most homogenous words selected in objec­ tive 1 research design a descriptive cross sectional design was adopted in this study (connolly, c, statistician, medical research council, per­ sonal communication, august 2009). such a design was consid­ ered suitable as it provided the data at a fixed point in time (maxwell & satake, 2006). a quantitative approach was followed because the study involved the analysis of responses to the audi­ bility of the words at different intensity levels. this was followed by an analysis of the prosodic patterns of the zulu words. the methodological framework for each objective is described below. objective 1: to determine which of the 58 prese­ lected zulu words met the criterion of homogeneity of audibility participant selection criteria the following participant selection criteria were adopted for this part of the study. qrhe participants had to be between the age of 18 25 years, as hearing sensitivity is judged to be at its peak during this age period (jerger, 1970). qrhe participants had to speak zulu as their first language and had to have been permanent residents of kzn for more than 10 years. this criterion ensured that the participants would be famil­ iar with the items selected for testing and that there would be no confounding dialectal factors. qrhe participants could have been male or female, as gender specificity was not investigated. the participants were required to have had no previous expo­ sure to industrial or recreational noise, no exposure to ototoxic drug consumption and must have reported no previous medical, neurological or acquired illnesses. further, participants had to have normal hearing. no family history of hearing loss should be reported. the above factors could contribute to auditory disorders that could confound the results. bess and humes (2003) de­ scribe the above factors as exogenous and endogenous factors, which could result from varying degrees of hearing loss. the above factors were evaluated using a case history questionnaire and were further confirmed by otoscopic examination, pure tone audiometry and immittance testing. the participants had to have no exposure to hearing testing before their participation in this study (robinson & koenings, 1979). this criterion was necessary in order to control for any learned effects regarding any of the behavioral measures used. sampling method purposive nonprobability sampling was used for this part of the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 63 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) seema panday, fiarsha kathard, mershen pitlay, cyril govender the study. zulu first language speakers attending the university of kwa -zulu natal were targeted as the study population. adver­ tisements for participants included posters and an email advert on the university lan. the advertisement included the specific criteria for participation in the study. thirty-two participants re­ sponded to the original advertisements. however, once the sam­ ple selection criteria and the hearing testing process via case history, otoscopic examination, pure tone audiometry and immit tance testing were completed, only thirty participants were con­ sidered suitable for the study. the two participants who did not meet the selection criteria were referred for further audiological testing and ent management. participants thirty normal hearing zulu first language speaking (zfls) stu­ dents from kzn were included as participants to realise this first objective of the study. the mean age of the participants was 21.5 years. there were 22 male and 8 female participants. thirty adults were considered adequate, as the minimum number used for the evaluation of homogeneity of audibility in other studies was reported to be between twenty and twenty five (nissen et al., 2005). materials and apparatus words fifty eight bisyllabic, low tone zulu verbs were measured for homogeneity of audibility. the process resulting in the selection of the fifty eight words is described by panday et al. (2007). bisyl­ labic low tone verbs were selected according to careful considera­ tion of zulu linguistic structure. furthermore, the 58 preselected words were subjected to analysis at two levels i.e. analysis in terms of commonality of the words in the zulu language, a linguis­ tic rating strategy in terms of familiarity and phonetic dissimilarity and tone (refer to panday et al., 2007). recordings in order to achieve objective 1, 58 preselected words had to be recorded onto compact discs (cd). the recording was done in a recording studio. the equipment and the procedure for the re­ cording was according to the guidelines stipulated by lyregaard (1997); asha (1988) and nissen et al. (2005) for recorded mate­ rials. the u87a p48 type neuman microphone was used for the recording. the akg cover/windshield was used over the micro­ phone to prevent distortion, especially when plosive sounds were produced. the microphone was positioned approximately 15cm from the speaker at 0 degree azimuth. the microphone was con­ nected to a sound mixer (mackie 32-8-2 8-bus mixing console) in the control room. the recordings made were normalized to odbhl levels. the recording of the words was preceded by a cali­ bration pure tone signal which is according to the recent iso 8253-3 specification for recorded materials. the calibration sig­ nal was weighted, using a frequency-modulated tone at 1 khz, which had a bandwidth of at least 1/3 octave (nissen et al., 2005). the modulating signal was sinusoidal and had a repetition rate of 4-20hz. the calibration tone lasted 60 seconds and was followed by an instruction read by a male zulu first language speaker. the zulu words were recorded in diverse, random se­ quences, on six different tracks on the cd. fifty-eight words were recorded on each of the six tracks. the six tracks were randomi­ zations of the same 58 words. the fifty-eight words were re­ corded with a 5 second pause between each word (lyregaard, 1997). each word was preceded by a carrier phrase “yithi” which means “say" in zulu. the zulu first language speaker was ad­ vised to read the words in the natural tone that the words are spoken in accordance with the dialect used in the durban pietermaritzburg region of kzn. the recordings were transferred from the mixer desk to the carilon audio system computer hard drive. the wavelab software package was used to edit the words. the words were normalized to peak at odb and were adjusted once all recordings were done. the editing software allowed for the words to have the same rms power as the 1000hz calibra­ tion tone in an initial attempt to equate all the words. the use of a constant rms allows for the output of the signal (the presenta­ tion of the words) to remain constant. this is a standard method of controlling any fluctuations in the signal and has been de­ scribed in other studies such as ramkissoon et al. (2002) and nissen et al. (2005). the recordings were then produced onto a cd using the nero start smart cd writer. the choice of speaker for the recording of the cd the speaker selected for the recording of the cd was a male zulu first language university student who resided in the durban pietermaritzburg region where zulu is the main language spoken. therefore, the dialect used in the recording was matched to that of the material selected and to the participants in this study. re­ cent research has indicated minimal gender influences on the recording of the material and the overall homogeneity of audibility of the words (cambron, wilson & shanks, 1991; nissen et al., 2005). j procedure ^ procedure for the measurement of homogeneity of audibility the measurement of homogeneity of audibility using the recorded words occurred in two sessions i.e. session one involved testing of the participants for normal hearing and session two involved the measurement of homogeneity of audibility. prior to any testing, the university of kwa zulu-natal ethics committee approved the study. informed consent was obtained from each participant before com­ mencing with testing. the testing of the participants for normal hearing was conducted using standard audiometric procedures. these procedures included a detailed case history, otoscopic exami­ nation, immittance audiometry, air conduction testing for frequen­ cies 125 hz to 8khz. an isolated industrial acoustics company (iac) twin audiometric-soundproof booth of double wall construc64 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the hom og en eity of au d ib ility and prosody of zulu w o rds for speech reception thr esh old testing tion was used as the test environment for the pure tone testing and speech evaluation. these requirements were set by the american national standards institute [ansi] (1977) to meet the noise level requirements. all equipment used in session one and two was calibrated prior to testing (june 2005). appendix a re­ flects the pure tone averages for all participants. based on the results of the basic battery in session one, the participants were selected or excluded from the study. the ex­ cluded participants were referred for further diagnostic testing and appropriate management at the local clinic, hospital or uni­ versity clinic. thirty participants had normal hearing based on the results of the basic battery and were therefore included in ses­ sion two i.e. for the measurement of homogeneity of audibility of the words. the participants were seated in the test room while the re­ searcher and two zulu first language speaking scorers were in the control room. each participant was instructed in zulu by a zulu first language speaker. the speaker was trained by the re­ searcher in terms of the instructions and procedure to be fol­ lowed. the instructions orientated the participants to the nature of the task. the instructions specified the participant’s mode of response and indicated that the test material was speech. in­ structions were given for participants to repeat the words that they heard verbally, including those words presented at the faint­ est listening levels. the verbal response is supported in the lit­ erature by lyregaard (1997). the scoring of participant’s re­ sponses was done independently by the researcher and the two zulu first language speaking scorers. the researcher included this scoring method in order to improve the reliability of the scor­ ing process. the 58 words (appendix b) were captured onto a scoring form developed by the researcher. the participant’s re­ sponse was scored with a tickjfor an accurate, and a cross for an inaccurate response or if there was no response. the researcher randomly selected the test ear because only one ear of each participant was tested for the homogeneity of audibility of the words. the fjarticipants had normal hearing in both ears and it did not matter which ear was selected for testing. j the participants were instructed verbally by a zulu first language speaker. thereafter, the recorded material was presented to each participant. the recording began with the 1000hz calibration tone, followed by a recorded instruction in zulu. the instructions were routed through the gsi 61 audiometer using tdh-49 earphones. the intensity level of the instruction was at 30dbhl. at the onset of testing for homogeneity of the words, the attenuator dial was set at odbhl. at this level one recording of 58 words was played to the participant. the number of correct and incorrect words was recorded by each of the three scorers on the scoring form. the attenuator was raised in 5db steps and a second recording was administered. a different recording was then played successively at each 5db increment until 100% identification was reached for all 58 words. the participants were given a 5 minute rest period after three recordings were played. the rest period was incorporated in order to improve co­ operation from all participants and to eliminate fatigue. further­ more, the participants were reinforced by the researcher through non-verbal reinforcements. all thirty participants were tested in the same way. procedure for analysis of homogeneity of audibility fifty eight bisyllabic words were played back via a cd player to the participants. the participants were instructed to repeat the words heard. there were 3 scorers used to assess the responses of the participants in terms of the audibility of the words. the fifty eight words were presented at different sound intensity levels viz. at 0, 5, 10, 15 and 20 decibels. the kappa test of agreement according to agresti (1990) was used to assess the consistency of the scorers. the kappa test was chosen since the responses were binary and it was a suitable test of agreement between two or more scorers. results indicated a good agreement amongst all three scorers at 0.05 level of agreement. the good inter-rater reliability across all three scorers allowed the researcher to choose the scores of one of the zulu first language scorers for application of the logistic regression equation. the use of the logistic regression equation led to the analysis of homogeneity of audibility. logistic regression was used since the data was binary (nominal) in nature. appendix c describes the motivation and procedure used regarding the logistic equations, as stipulated by agresti (1990). this was done using computer­ ized statistical analysis i.e. spss version 11.6. a statistician at the medical research council of south africa (durban) assisted with the statistical analysis. the average percentage correct iden­ tified by each participant for each word was calculated using the above equation. the performance intensity curve for the most homogenous words was identified. the steepness of the curve was calculated using a logistic regression plot .the raw scores for the 58 words were inserted into a logistic regression equation that was designed to calculate the percentage correct at each intensity level. using the regression equation, the calculations for the range of threshold (50%) and the slope of the curve was as­ certained for each word. this model provided the researcher with a method of estimating, at any given intensity, the performance of the words in terms of percentage. the most homogenous words were selected and the cd recording was edited. this con­ sisted of 28 words. these words were then considered for the acoustic analysis of the prosodic features outlined in objective two of the study. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 65 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) seema panday, harsha kathard, mershen pillay, cyril govender objective 2: to describe the prosodic features, spe­ cifically the pitch and energy contours of the most homogenous words selected in objective one sample material the twenty-eight zulu words identified as being most homoge­ nous were included as material for this objective of the study. the recorded words were used for the acoustic analysis. apparatus and choice of tests the multispeech model 3700 by kay-elemetrics corporation was the most suitable apparatus for the present investigation. it is a digitally driven system. the accompanying software pro­ grams enable the system to analyze the pitch contours of the signal, determine the fundamental frequency of the input signal and measure the spectrographic waveform patterns of the speech signal. procedure the input signal for the multispeech 3700 analysis was via the cd recording. the multispeech main program software was used. four display window settings were created. display a represented the digitized sound wave, display b was a wideband spectro­ graphic analysis, and c and d represented the pitch contour and energy contours respectively. however, for the purposes of this study the pitch and energy contour settings were most relevant. each of the 28 words was recorded on the cd and was analyzed acoustically. there was a separate pitch contour and energy con­ tour for each word, which allowed for ease of analysis and com­ parison. analysis all 28 words were carefully analysed for the acoustic properties that were relevant for this study. the following parameters were used: pitch contours energy contours the pitch contours and energy contours of each syllable within a word were analyzed via visual inspection. this entailed identify­ ing the highest points for pitch and spectral energy for each sylla­ ble of the word, by placing the cursor on the highest point of the contour and reading the values. thereafter, differences in the pitch and overall spectral energy for each syllable were deter­ mined using descriptive statistics. data was tabulated. the differ­ ence values for pitch and energy were calculated as such values could be used to confirm the prosodic features of zulu (e. gro enewald, lecturer, university of pretoria personal communica­ tion, october 2006). the use of pitch was particularly important as the prosodic features in a language are usually indicated by the rise and fall in the pitch of the word (kent & read, 1992). furthermore, the similarities in the pitch and energy content of the words could confirm the homogeneity of the words acousti66 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, cally. the results of the study are outlined below. results the results are presented according to each of the two objec­ tives outlined in the method. results for the measurement of homogeneity of au­ dibility the data for the homogeneity of audibility was treated statisti­ cally using the logistic regression analysis technique. the esti­ mates of the regression model are indicated in table 1. all slopes and intercepts were significant at the 0.05 level of significance. the calculations of the threshold intensity and slope are also provided. these values are calculated using the equations out­ lined in appendix c. table 2 indicates the slope obtained at 50%, the slope at 20 to 80% and the estimated threshold obtained according to the regression model for each of the words. table 1. estimates of the regression model word a p word a p banga -2.393 0.227 linda -2.267 0.24 bheka -1.351 0.179 loya -3.017 0.237 bhema -1.586 0.173 lunga -3.856 0.262 chela -3.014 0.307 minya -3.662 0.237 cinga -2.91 0.258 pheka -1.897 0.162 dansa -1.974 0.222 phonsa -2.001 0.175 dinga -2.7 0.133 qoba -1.105 0.203 donsa -2.165 0.234 sefa -1.817 0.077 faka -0.283 0.257 shada -1.978 0.304 finya -3.268 0.175 shaya -0.433 0.186 geza -1.85 0.228 thanda -1.729 0.156 goba -1.827 0.168 thatha -2.689 0.242 gonda -2.578 0.617 thela -2.877 0.271 gqoka 0.104 0.167 thenga -2.871 0.353 gxeka -1.121 0.3 thola -2.313 0.439 hlala 0.921 0.214 vala -1.199 0.216 hleka -1.205 0.19 veza -3.942 0.273 hlenga -2.347 0.226 vula -2.794 0.207 hluba -2.587 0.193 vuma -2.527 0.195 hola -2.524 0.178 washa -1.109 0.383 jaha -2.197 0.22 wina -3.401 0.26 jeza -2.94 0.322 xola -2.262 0.321 khaba -2.013 0.288 yanga -1.908 0.261 khanya -1.385 0.229 yeba -2.566 0.261 kheta -2.227 0.26 yeka -2.061 0.316 khipa -2.581 0.254 yenza -2.222 0.214 landa -1.377 0.178 yifa -2.024 0.154 letha -2.334 0.206 yona -2.94 0.322 lima -2.288 0.158 yosa -3.489 0.2 „ 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the homogeneity of audibility and prosody of zulu words for speech reception threshold testing the data recorded in table 2 indicates that on average the is greatest at this point. for a unit change in x (intensity level) we see estimated threshold intensity (softest level) at which all the words the largest increase in the probability of audibility (p(x) = 1). the measwere heard was 9.66dbhl. on average the 50% correct recogniurement of homogeneity of the word list is dependent on those chosen tion point occurred at about 9.66dbhl. the slope at 50% was an to have a steep threshold slope, important statistic to calculate, since it represents the steepest slope on the logistic curve. this indicates that the rate of change table 2. summary of the slope at 50%; slope at 20 to 80% and the estimated threshold. word slope at 50% (0.5*0.5*b) *100 slope at 20 or 80%: (0.2*0.8*b)*100 (-)a/b = threshold word slope at 50% (0.5*0.5*b) *100 slope at 20 or 80%: (0.2*0.8*b)*100 (-)a/b = threshold banga 5.675 3.632 10.542 linda 6 3.84 9.446 bheka 4.475 2.864 7.547 loya 5.925 3.792 12.73 bhema 4.325 2.768 9.168 lunga 6.55 4.192 14.718 chela 7.675 4.912 9.818 minya 5.925 3.792 15.451 cinga 6.45 4.128 11.279 pheka 4.05 2.592 11.71 dansa 5.55 3.552 8.892 phonsa 4.375 2.8 11.434 dinga 3.325 2.128 20.301 qoba 5.075 3.248 5.443 donsa 5.85 3.744 9.252 sefa 1.925 1.232 23.597 faka 6.425 4.112 1.101 shada 7.6 4.864 6.507 finya 4.375 2.8 18.674 shaya 4.65 2.976 2.328 geza 5.7 3.648 8.114 thanda 3.9 2.496 11.083 goba 4*2 2.688 10.875 thatha 6.05 3.872 11.112 gonda 15.425 9.872 4.178 thela 6.775 4.336 10.616 gqoka 4.175 2.672 -0.623 thenga 8.825 5.648 8.133 gxeka 7.5 4.8 3.737 thola 10.975 7.024 5.269 hlala 5.35 i 3.424 -4.304 vala 5.4 3.456 5.551 hleka 4.75 ! 3.04 6.342 veza 6.825 4.368 14.44 hlenga 5.65 i 3.616 10.385 vula 5.175 3.312 13.498 hluba 4.825 3.088 13.404 vuma 4.875 3.12 12.959 hola 4.45 2.848 14.18 washa 9.575 6.128 2.896 iaha 5.5 ' 3.52 9.986 wina 6.5 4.16 13.081 ieza 8.05 ! 5.152 9.13 xola 8.025 5.136 7.047 khaba 7.2 ■" 4.608 6.99 yanga 6.525 4.176 7.31 khanva 5.725 3.664 6.048 yeba 6.525 4.176 9.831 kheta 6.5 4.16 8.565 yeka 7.9 5.056 6.522 khipa 6.35 4.064 10.161 yenza 5.35 3.424 10.383 landa 4.45 2.848 7.736 yifa 3.85 2.464 13.143 letha 5.15 3.296 11.33 yona 8.05 5.152 9.13 lima 3.95 2.528 14.481 yosa 5 3.2 17.445 summary statistics slope at 50% slope at 20 or 80%: threshold intensity mean 5.986 3.831 9.657 standard deviation 2.037 1.304 4.85 maximum , 15.425 9.872 23.597 minimum 1.925 1.232 -4.304 range 13.5 8.64 27.901 the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 67 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) seema panday, harsha kathard, mershen pillay, cyril govender figure 1. performance intensity curves for zulu words finally selected o o level (db) -----bhema ----finya -----goba — hlala — hlaka -----hlenga -----hluba — hola -----jaha -----kheta -----landa -----letha -----lima -----loya -----phonsa -----sefa ----vuma ----yifa ----yosa ----average of all ----bheka ----dansa ----gqoka ----dinga ----lunga ----pheka ----qoba ----shaya ----thanda ----vula ----yenza figure 2. performance intensity curves for zulu words not selected 0 . 7 5 0 . 5 0 . 2 5 5 2 . 5 0 2 . 5 5 7 . 5 1 0 1 2 . 3 5 1 7 . 2 0 2 2 . 2 5 --------c i n g a --------g x e ka --------b a n g a ------c h e la ------g e z a ------j e z a ------kh an ya ------k h ip a ------k h e t a ------i in da ------m i n y a ------l o y a ■------s ha d a ------th ath a ------th el a ------th en ga ------th ol a ------w as h a ------w i na ------x o l a ------k h a b a ------fa ka ------y o n g a ------do ns a ------y o n a 1 -----a v e r a g e o f a l l ------y e k a \ ------qo nd a v e za ! l e v e l s (d b ) the mean slope at 50% was found to be 5.986%/db (sd= 2.037). therefore, the words that were above the mean slope were considered most homogenous. these words had the steep­ est slope and satisfied the criterion of homogeneity of audibility. twenty eight words were therefore considered fairly homogenous on the basis of the above analysis. the performance intensity curve of the 28 words is described in figurel. a description of individual performance curves is available from the authors. fig­ ure 2 illustrates the performance intensity curves for the words not selected. the mean slope of the words selected as indicated in figure 1 was 5.9865%/db. this figure also clearly indicates that the words selected had the steepest slope compared to the words not selected in figure 2. figure 2 indicates that the slope at 50% for each of the words was more gradual. the gradual slope indicates that for these words to be identified correctly the words had to be presented at higher intensity levels compared to the words identified as being most homogenous results emerging from the analysis of the prosodic features of the words the twenty eight words selected in objective 1 were analyzed / acoustically for their prosodic features. table 3 indicates the re­ sults obtained for each of the twenty eight words. the majority of the words (89%) indicate a relatively lower pitch value in the first syllable compared to the second syllable. the pitch values in table 3 also indicate a difference in pitch between syllable one and two. the magnitude of the difference value serves to con68 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the homogeneity of audibility and prosody of zulu words for speech reception threshold testing firm the change in pitch between syllable one and two. this conthe penultimate syllable, and therefore prominence is noted in firms the presence of tonal patterns in the selected zulu verbs the second syllable. fifty eight bisyllabic low tone verbs were measured for homogeneity of audibility and were then further sub­ jected to an acoustic analysis for the description of prosodic features. twenty eight words were most homoge­ nous, because they had the steepest perform ance curves. statistically, words that were identified with a greater than 5.98%/db slope, were considered to be most homogenous. the performance curve is used to illus­ trate how the words were heard at different intensity levels. the critical element of the performance curve is the steepness or the slope of the curve. the mean slope of these twenty eight words was 5.98%/db. the mean slope, therefore, is comparable to that of other languages and this is indicated in table 4 below. figure 1 also indicates variability in the curves generated. however, closer inspection of the twenty eight words selected (see figure 1 in the results section) illustrates that the majority of the curves appear similar in shape with a distinct sshaped curve. in contrast, however, the remaining 30 words were very different in shape (see figure 2 in the results section). these table 3. pitch and energy values: syllable 1 and 2 i the majority of the words (89%) show a similar pattern regard­ ing pitch, and this serves to confirm the presence of prosodic patterns in zulu. eleven percent (3/28) did not indicate this pat­ tern. this finding could have been influenced by the speaker plac­ ing artificial stress on the first syllable, similar to that when a spondee word is uttered. this limitation was expected as the speaker was a student of audiology and the common use of spon­ dee words in clinical practice may have influenced this produc­ tion. the energy values of the vowels in each syllable also indi­ cated a difference between syllable one and two. generally, a higher energy value was noted in the second syllable of the words with the exception of seven words. the difference in the energy values obtained may also be-due to the prosodic features of zulu, whereby in isolation, low tone verbs may indicate lengthening of the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 69 words indicated a more gradual slope than the words consid­ ered as most homogenous. the gradual slopes indicate that for these words to be identified correctly the words had to be pre­ sented at higher intensity levels compared to the words identi­ fied as being most homogenous. the results obtained in the present study, when compared to studies of other languages indicate both similarities and differ­ ences. table 4 provides a summary of the data obtained across different studies regarding the homogeneity of the word lists. i.e. low tone verbs. discussion no word pitch value syllable 1 pitch value syllable 2 difference in pitch between syllables energy value syllable 1 energy value syllable 2 difference in energy values between syllables 1 banga 107 113 6 67.83 73.9 10.1 2 gxeka 107 144 37 69.75 73 3.5 3 cinga 176 125 51 73.91 67.4 6.5 4 khanya 81 106 25 68.05 67.01 0.9 5 thela 106 135 29 60.3 68.8 8.5 6 khaba 108 132 24 69.12 75.4 6.2 7 kheta 104 125 21 55.1 60.9 5.8 8 jeza 79 152 73 69 69.18 0.2 9 donsa 139 93 46 69.9 69.31 0.6 10 khipa 85 121 36 66.38 72 5.6 11 chela 82 130 48 60.25 74.06 13.6 12 xola 109 131 22 66.64 60.3 1.3 13 yonga 88 104 16 70,28 69.26 1 14 yona 88 104 16 66.26 67.6 1.3 15 linda 87 98 11 65.91 66.08 0.8 16 veza 91 97 6 68.63 75.3 6.7 17 loya 106 127 21 72.06 63.6 8.5 18 thenga 151 141 10 60.2 46.1 14.2 19 wina 108 136 27 52.3 61.2 8.9 20 faka 81 106 25 68.63 75.8 72 21 washa 87 98 11 66.65 67.6 0.9 22 yeka 82 130 48 66.38 72 5.6 23 minya 105 151 46 49.2 56.9 7.7 24 thola 96 130 34 66.64 70.81 4.2 25 shada , 85 121 36 70.26 74.81 4.6 25 shada ' 85 121 36 70.26 74.81 4.6 26 thata j 85 127 42 68.63 74.81 6.2 27 qonda jl0 7 139 32 69.75 72.81 3.1 28 geza 1109 146 37 66.71 69.1 7.4 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) seema panday, harsha kathard, mershen pillay, cyril govender 'researcher/s language type of stimuli and number of words evaluated scale of measurement mean slope @ 50% present study zulu bisyllabic verbs (28) dbhl 5.98%/db (6%/db) hudgins et al. (1947) english bisyllabic s p o n d e e s (36) db 7.2%/db young et al. (1982) english bisyllabic spondees (15) dbspl 10%/db c ardenas & marrero (1994) spanish trisyllables and fo u r syllables (24) db 8%/db c hristensen (1995), as cited in nissen e t al. (2005) spanish trisyllables (12) db 11.1 %/db g reer (1997), as cited in nissen e t al. (2005) italian trisyllabic db 7.3%/db ~ nissen et al. (2005) m andarin t risyllabic (24) db 11.3%/db nissen e t al. (2008) taiwan m andarin bisyllabic db 9.6/db table 4. overview of the mean slope values across studies. table 4 indicates that there are differences in the slope at 50%/ db across languages. the mean slope at 50% for the studies that utilized trisyllables compared to the bisyllables appear to be steeper e.g. 11.3%/db in the nissen et al. (2005) study compared to the 6%/db in the present study. the steepness of the curve, however, is directly influenced by the type of stimuli used (kruger & kruger, 1997). in addition, the number of syllables in the stimuli influences the steepness of the slope. trisyllabic or polysyllabic words are more redundant than bisyllabic words. redundancy in speech audiometry is defined as “ the abundance of information available to the listener due to substantial content of the speech material and the capacity of information inherent in richly inner­ vated pathways of the central system”( stach, 2003 p. 227). thus, the abundance of information present within the speech signals of trisyllables or polysyllables allows for the words to be heard at softer levels, contributing to a steeper slope (stach, 1998). furthermore, while the concept of redundancy is not highlighted as a criterion in the selection of srt materials, stach (1998) warns that the more redundant the stimuli is, the more immune the sig­ nal is to detecting a hearing loss. therefore, the current research­ ers maintain the argument in support of using less redundant stim­ uli in the present study, i.e. bisyllabic zulu words, as they may be more sensitive than trisyllabic or polysyllabic words in determining srt. notwithstanding the differences obtained in the present study compared to other languages, the aim of determining which of the 58 preselected zulu words met the criterion of homogeneity of audibility was realized in this investigation. an accurate replica of scores for the slope at 50% between and among studies is not possible, as languages used are different and the methodologies followed were different in determining the slope at 50%. another explanation for the more gradual slopes obtained in this study compared to the earlier studies conducted in english could be related to the differences in the linguistic structure of the lan­ guages. the use of spondees with equal stress explains the ease of identification at low intensity levels. brandy (2002) confirms this by stating that the performance intensity function for spondaic words is very steep. the average level for 100% correct recognition to be obtained would usually occur at 27.5dbspl (7.5dbhl). this is supported by earlier statements made by egan (1948) that spon­ dees have the highest homogeneity of audibility in comparison to other stimuli. it has also influenced subsequent researchers i.e. ashoor & proshazka,1985; plant, 1990; harris et al. 2001; nissen et al. 2005 to confirm homogeneity of audibility in their studies by drawing comparisons that are very closely related to the spondee words in english. the criterion to assess the steepness of the slope for languages that do not have spondees e.g. zulu, may however need to be al­ tered. this is based on the fact that the zulu words are different in stress pattern and structure to those of english, so the reference data used to evaluate the steepness of the slope should also be different. there is a strong possibility that the normal hearing zulu first language speakers in the present study may have had difficulty identifying the bisyllabic words in zulu due to the lack of promi­ nence of the first syllable, but required the intensity of the signal to be slightly higher for 100% recognition. this could, therefore, serve as a possible explanation for the mean threshold intensity for 100% correct recognition in the present study being 9dbhl. in addition, the morphological structure of the bisyllabic verbs usually has a /-a/ bound to the verb root (rycroft & ncgobo,,1979). i the presence of the /-a/ sound in the second syllable together with the prominent high tone may account for the poor recognition of the first syllable. thus, under difficult listening conditions, recogni­ tion of the first syllable may have been compromised, because of the lack of similar acoustic and morphological cues in the first syllable compared to the second syllable (chetty, 1990). scrutiny of the performance intensity curves for the zulu words indicates that the words that had the click sound jn -th e first sylla­ ble e.g. /qonda/ and /gxeka/ had the steepest slopes at 50% i.e. 15.425%/db and 7.5 %/db respectively. these click sounds carry a higher spectral energy content acoustically, and thus, could account for the greater recognition of these words at the lower intensity levels. however, the words such as /finya; dinga; lima/ 70 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) the hom ogeneity of a ud ib ility and prosod y of zulu w ords for speech reception thr esh old testing appeared to have had more gradual slopes at 50%. these words may be regarded as softer due to the fricatives and lower inten­ sity vowel sound /i/ being used. the variation in the acoustical energy content of the consonants and vowels of zulu may there­ fore have implications for the steepness of the slope. chetty (1990) briefly alludes to the influence of the acoustic properties of vowels and consonants on the steepness of the curve. within the present study, it would appear that the acoustic properties of the vowel and phoneme combinations could have contributed to the slope patterns observed. apart from chetty (1990), previous researchers have not referred to the influence of the energy con­ tent on the steepness of the slope. however, the present re­ searchers are of the opinion that for a language like zulu, the argument is relevant and worthy of noting. this point introduces a new lens through which homogeneity with respect to audibility is viewed in languages where spondees are not available. further, qualitative comments made by participants indicated that they often heard the second syllable at the lower intensities, but had difficulty with hearing the first syllable. this reinforces the possi­ ble influence of prosody on the steepness of the slope. apart from the possible lower acoustic energy of the first sylla­ ble, the actual recording of the materials could have also affected the performance of the listeners. nissen et al. (2005) explain the importance of high quality digital recording in the development of materials for srt. while every attempt was made to adhere to this recommendation in the present study, the recording of the words in terms of vu meter peaking were +/-2db across the words. manual adjustments had to be introduced to the vu set­ tings between the calibration tone and the words. furthermore, the six randomizations were individually recorded due to a lack of i software to help to randomize the recordings automatically. i these limitations in recording, could have possibly affected the performance of the listeners with regard to the recognition of the words (refer to panday (2006) for further explanation about the recording process in this investigation). i despite the resource constraints and time limitations of this study, all data obtained were verified at several levels of analysis and therefore, remain reliable. perhaps an implication of this would be to re-record the most homogenous words after introduc­ ing an intensity adjustment digitally. this has been done histori­ cally and more recently by nissen et al. (2005): the variability of the curves is reduced and the steepness of the curves is im­ proved. this could also account for the reason why the slopes obtained in the nissen et al. (2005) study were similar to the data obtained in the original english studies. from a methodological perspective, homogeneity of audibility has been assessed using different mathematical, statistical and procedural methods. young et al. (1982) have raised the histori­ cal discrepancies noted in the english word lists regarding homo­ geneity of audibility. these researchers articulated concerns re­ garding the definition of homogeneity. earlier studies (bowling & elpern, 1961; curry & cox, 1966; beattie et al. 1975) reported on the intensity level at which words were first identified correctly, and made no provisions in their analysis if the word was missed in subsequent presentations (young et al., 1982). in addition, the rate at which words were intelligible was not specified in the ear­ lier studies. the present study considered these earlier limita­ tions and utilized a more stringent mathematical model to calcu­ late accurately the threshold of intensity and the slope of the psychometric function, taking into account the performance of participants across all lists. in addition, the rate at which words became intelligible can be easily calculated using this model. the model has also been described by nissen etal.(2005). while the present study followed a 5db change in the intensity levels, the studies discussed above, including the nissen et al. (2005) study used a 2db intensity change. the use of the 5db intensity change was adopted so as to maintain a consistent pattern with the manner in which srt is usually established (gelfand, 1997). the difference in intensity change, however, could account for the more gradual slope in the present study. the performance intensity curve with 2db levels would have more reference points for calculating percentage correct responses. the steepness of these curves would certainly improve the overall slope and shape of the curve. future studies should con­ sider decreasing the increments between the different intensity levels, thus ensuring that many more levels are assessed. another issue to be considered is the learning effect. young et al. (1982) articulated concerns regarding the learning effect that could unfold if the same words are presented with several ran­ domizations at the many reference levels discussed above. while the use of randomization is considered to eliminate the learning effect, one could also argue that with 13 presentations of the same words to a participant, there is a strong possibility that the participants learn the words and their performance would there­ fore appear better. this could also account for the excellent per­ formance noted in the nissen et al. (2005) study. given the above, careful consideration must be afforded to the choice of intensity level changes and the number of randomizations se­ lected. while the possibility of improving the performance curves exists, the potential impact of ‘the learning effect’ requires fur­ ther investigation. in summary, it is clear that selecting homogenous words in any language presents a challenge. several factors can influence and affect the outcome. these may well include the linguistic, meth­ odological and procedural variables. these challenges reinforce the need for the adaptation of the original criteria for srt, at many levels, when materials are developed and selected for srt testing. the reference data for evaluating homogeneity of audibil­ ity in english simply cannot be applied stringently to that of zulu the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 71 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) seema panday, harsha kathard, mershen pillay, cyril govender or any other language. the concept of homogeneity of audibility is achievable in any language, but the criterion used to select the most homogenous words must suit that language. this study described the selected words acoustically via an acoustic analysis system. the information obtained for the pitch contours is relevant to the present study. the majority of the words analyzed (25/28), indicated a similar pitch contour i.e. there was a difference in the pitch between the first and sec­ ond syllable. syllable one was consistently lower in pitch than syllable two. these findings follow the prosodic patterns of the zulu words selected i.e. low tone verbs. similarly the energy con­ tours of the vowel sound in each word indicated differences be­ tween the two syllables. however, the patterns for 3/28 words indicated that the first syllable was higher in pitch than the sec­ ond syllable. this finding could have been influenced by the speaker placing artificial stress on the first syllable, similar to a spondee word. a possible recommendation for future research would be to ensure that several recordings of the words are made and judged by listeners of the language before the final recording is accepted. this is supported by nissen et al. (2005). nevertheless, the overall similarity between the pitch contours and energy contours for the majority of the words confirm that not only are the words homogenous in terms of linguistic and audiological variables, but most of the words were acoustically homogenous too. in addition, the use of the acoustic analysis to confirm the prosodic features in the language may be seen as an additional method for selecting words for srt testing. this ap­ proach was clearly supported by plant (1990), whereby spectro graphic analysis supported the stress patterns of trisyllabic words in tiwi. the application of acoustic analysis in the development of speech materials is in its infancy. however, the findings of the present study support the inclusion of acoustic analysis together with measurement of homogeneity of audibility as suitable ‘tools' to be used in the selection of words for srt testing. conclusion the findings in this investigation have important implications, both clinically and in terms of future research in the field. the constitution of the republic of south africa (act 200 of 1993), as amended by act 109 of 1996, clearly confirms the importance of the eleven official languages of the country and the linguistic rights of individuals. in this context, the present investigation could be seen as a step towards the selection of srt materials that are appropriate, relevant and scientifically designed to cater for the audiological needs of zulu first language speakers in kzn. similar research needs to be considered for the other official languages in south africa, apart from english. in terms of the diagnostic significance, it is clear that the meas­ urement of srt is fundamental to the initial audiological process. however, the value of the test is lost when the materials used are not appropriately selected. in fact, the danger of over and under­ diagnosis is evident. thus, research of this nature provides the necessary steps towards improving the service delivery for zulu first language clients in kzn. however, while the selection of an appropriate srt word list was achieved in this study, the data obtained is limited to the adult normative population. hence, there is a need for future research in terms of the wider clinical population. references agresti, a. (1990). categorical data analysis. new york, usa: john wiley sons. american national standards institute (1977). standard criteria for perm issibility o f am bient noise during audiom etric testing (ansi s31977)new york. american national standards institute (1996). am erican national specification for audiom eters (ansi s3.6-1996). new york. american speech-language-hearing association (1988). guide­ lines for determining the threshold for speech. asha, 3, 85-88. ashoor, a. a., & prochazka, t. (1985). saudi arabic speech audi­ ometry in children. british journal o f audiology, 19, 229-238. balkisson, y. (2001). speech discrimination testing: towards the developm ent o f wordlists for zulu-first language speaking chil­ dren. unpublished undergraduate research report. university of durban-westville, durban, south africa. beattie, r. c., svihovee, b. c., & edgerton, b. j. (1975). relative intelligibility of the cid spondees as presented via monitored live-voice. journal o f speech and hearing disorders, 40, 84 91. bess, f. h. & humes, f. (2003). audiology the fundamentals. usa: lippincott williams & wilkins. borg, e., wilson, m., & samuelson, e. (1998). towards an ecologi­ cal audiology: stereophonic listening chamber and acoustic environmental tests. scandinavian audiology, 2 7 , 195-206. bowling, l. s., & elpern, b. s. (1961). relative intelligibility of items on c.i.d. auditory test w -l. journal o f auditory research, 1, 152-157. brandy, w. t. (2002). speech audiometry. in j. katz (ed.), hand­ book o f | clinical audiology. (pp. 96-110). baltimore: williams and wilkins. i cambron, n. k., wilson, r. h. & shanks, j. e. (1991). spondaic word detection and recognition functions for female and'male speakers. ear & hearing, 12, 64-70. cardenas, m. r. & marrero, v. (1994). speech audiom etry in spanish: new recorded lists. proceeding of the 20th interna­ tional congress of audiology, halifax. chermack, g. d.,& musiek, e. f. (1997). central au ditory process­ ing disorders new perspectives. london: singular publishers group. / chetty, s. (1990). an investigation into the effectiveness o f two zulu word lists in establishing speech reception thresholds in norm al hearing bilingual (english/zulu) subjects. unpublished undergraduate dissertation. university of durban-westville, durban, south africa. cope, a.t. (1982). a com prehensive course in the zulu language. 72 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) th e hom og en eity of a ud ib ility and prosody of zulu w o rd s for speech reception thr esh old testing durban howard college, university of natal. curry, e.t., & cox, b.p. (1966). the relative intelligibility of items on cid auditory test w l. journal o f auditory research, 6, 419 424. demody, p. & lee, k. (1997). speech tests at the national acous­ tic laboratories. in m. martin (ed.), speech audiom etry (pp. 297-314). london: whurr publishers. doke, c. m. (1930). textbook o f zulu grammar. rsa: longman green company ltd. egan, j.p. (1948). articulation testing methods. laryngoscope, 58, 955-991. fox, a. (2000). prosody. new york: oxford university press. gelfand, s. a. (1997). essentials o f audiology. new york: thieme medical publishers. grimes, j. (1992). ucla language materials zulu language pro­ file. retrieved november 02, 2004, from http:// www.lmd.ucla.edu/drofiles/drofz01.htm harris, r. w., goffi, m. v. s., pedalini, m. e. b., gygi, m. a., & merrill, a. (2001). psychometically equivalent brazilianportu­ guese bisyllabic word recognition materials spoken by male and female talkers. profono, 13, 249-262. harris, r.w., kim, e.& eggett, d.l. (2003). psychometrically equivalent korean bisyllabic words spoken by male and fe­ male talkers. korean journal o f communication disorders, 8, 217-337. harris, r. w., nielson,w.s. mcpherson,d.l., skarzynski,h.& eg­ gett,d.l. (2004). psychometrically equivalent polish bisyllabic words spoken by male and female talkers. audiofonologia, 25, 1-15 hirsh, i.j., silverman, s. r., reynolds, e. g., eldert, e., & benson, r.w. (1952). development of materials for speech audiometry. journal o f speech and hearing disorders, 17, 332-337. hudgins, c.v., hawkins, j. e., karlin, j. e., & stevens, s. s. (1947). the development of recorded auditory tests for measuring hear­ ing loss for speech. laryngoscope, 57, 57-89. i international organization for standardization (iso) 8253-3 (1996). acousticaudiom etric test methods. part 3: speech audiometry: geneva. j jacobson, m.& trail, a. (1986). assessment of speech intelligibil­ ity in south eastern bantu languages: critical considerations. sajcd, 33, 15-28. ; jerger, j. (1970). clinical experience with impedance audiometry. archives o f otolaryngology, 92, 311-324. kent, r. d.,& read, c. (1992). the acoustic analysis o f speech. london: singular publishing group. knight, j. (1997). speech tests in some languages other than english. in m. martin (ed.), speech audiom etry: (pp 315-324). london: whurr publishers. konkle, d. f., & rintlemen, w. f. (1985). principles o f speech audiometry. baltimore: university park press. kruger, b. & kruger, f. m. (1997). speech audiometry in usa. in m. martin (ed.), speech audiom etry (pp. 233-277). london: whurr publishers. lau, c. c., & so, k. w. (1988); material for cantonese speech audiometry constructed by appropriate phonetic principles. british journal of audiology, 22, 297-304. leedy, p. d., & ormrod, j. e. (2005). practical research. new jersey: prentice hall. lyregaard, p. (1997). towards a theory of speech audiometry tests. in m. martin (ed.), speech audiometry (pp. 34-62). london: whurr publishers. maxwell, d.l. & satake, e. (2006). research and statistical methods in communication sciences and disorders. canada: thomson delmar learning. nissen, s.l., harris, r. w., jennings, l. j., eggett, d. l., & buck, h. (2005). psychometrically equivalent trisyllabic words for speech reception threshold testing in mandarin. international journal of audiology, 44, 391-399. nissen, s.l., harris, r.w & dukes, a. (2008). word recognition materials for native speakers of taiwan mandarin. american journal of audiology, 17, 68-79. panday, s (2006). the development of a speech reception thresh­ old test for zulu first language speakers in kwa-zulu natal. masters thesis. university of kwa-zulu-natal. durban, south africa. panday, s.; kathard, h.; pillay, m.; & govender, c (2007) the devel­ opment of a zulu speech reception threshold test for zulu first language speakers in kwa zulu-natal. the south african journal of communication disorders, 54,111-122. plant, g. (1990). the development of speech tests in aboriginal languages. australian journal of audiology, 13, 30-40. punch, j. l., & howard, m.t. (1985). spondee recognition threshold as a function of set size. journal of speech, language & hearing disorders, 50,120-125. ramkissoon, i. (2000). speech reception thresholds for non-native speakers of english: digit pairs vs spondee words. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of illinois, urbana champaign, illinois. ramkissoon, i., proctor, a., lansing, c. & bilger, r. c. (2002). digit speech recognition threshold (srt) for non-native speakers of english. american journal of audiology, 11, 23-28. robinson, d. d., & koenings, m. j. (1979). a comparison of procedures and materials for speech reception thresholds. journal o f am erican a u d io lo g y society, 4, 227-230. roeser.j. r., valente, m., & horsford-dunn, h. (2000). audiology diagnosis. usa: thieme medical publishers. rycroft, d. k., & ngcobo, a. b. (1979). say it in zulu. pietermarizburg:university of natal press. schill, s. (1985). threshold for speech. in j. katz (ed.), handbook of clinical audiology. baltimore: williams & wilkins. silman, s., & silverman, c. a. (1991). audiology diagnosis. principles and practice. new york: academic press. i stach, b. a. (1998). clinical audiology: an introduction. san diego: singular publishing. stach, b. a. (2003). comprehensive dictionary of audiology llus trated. canada: library of congress. wilson, r.h., zizz, c.a., shanks, j. e., & cuasey, g. d. (1990). norma­ tive data in quiet broadband noise, and competing message for northwestern university auditory test no.6 by a female speaker. journal of speech hearing disorders, 55, 771-778. young, j.r., dudley, b., & gunter, m. b. (1982). thresholds and psy­ chometric functions of the individual spondaic words. journal of speech and hearing research, 25, 586-593. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 73 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) http://www.lmd.ucla.edu/drofiles/drofz01.htm seema panday, harsha kathard, mershen pillay, cyril govender appendix a pure tone average scores n pta(right) pta(left) 1 .00 .00 2 5.00 .00 3 13.30. 8.30 4 .00 .00 5 6.67 6.67 6 .00 .00 7 .00 .00 8 .00 -5.00 9 .00 .00 10 .00 -5.00 11 15.00 .00 12 .00 10.00 13 5.00 5.00 14 6.67 3.33 15 .00 .00 16 .00 .00 17 5.00 6.67 18 .00 .00 19 .00 .00 20 .00 .00 21 .00 .00 22 3.33 .00 23 5.00 5.00 24 3.33 5.00 25 13.33. 11.67 26 5.00 5.00 27 5.00 5.00 28 5.00 5.00 29 0.00 0.00 30 5.00 5.00 some basic background on the logistic model: since the binomial distribution is used, we might expect that there will be a relationship between logistic regression and chi square analysis. it turns out that the 2x2 contingency analyses with chi-square is really just a special case of logistic regression. with chi-square contingency analysis, the independent variable is dichotomous and the dependent variable is dichotomous. logistic analysis does not restrict the independent variable to be dichoto­ mous. generally when one uses binary data the linear regression is not an appropriate model at all, in fact the data usually takes on an s-shaped curve. our statistical effort to transform the s-shaped curve to a linear one requires us to employ the logistic distribu­ tion. figure: 1. s-shaped linear approximation to logistic re­ gression curve. for a binary response y and a quantitative explanatory variable x, we allow p(x) to show the probability of a success when x takes on any of its values for example in our data let x represent the sound intensity levels and y denote the success or failure (audible or not respectively.) then when x = 10, mathematically we write, ap p end ix b: fifty eight words considered for assessment banga hlala loya veza bheka hleka lunga vula bhema hlenga minya vuma chela hluba pheka washa cinga hola phonsa wina dansa jaha qoba xola dinga jeza sefa yanga donsa khaba shada yeba faka khanya shaya yeka finya kheta thanda yenza geza khipa thatha yifa goba landa thela yona gonda letha thenga yosa gqoka lima thola gxeka linda vala appendix c motivation for logistic regression logistic regression: p(x) = prob(y|x). read, the probability of y given x. we also define the odds of success as: p ( x ) odds = l p ( x ) the log of the odds is known as the logit. the simple logistic model is of the form, (for only one explanatory variable, x): logit[/>(x)] = log (i) / p(x) n lp ( x ) = a + fix this transformation, namely the logit (log of the odds), yields a linear function of the explanatory variables x. estimation of the 74 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasie-afwykings, vol, 56, 2009 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) parameters of interest in the logistic regression is done similarly to linear regression once the transformation has been applied. that is, we will have estimates for a and (5 which represent the intercept and slope respectively (these are generally calculated by some software package). solving for p(x) in equation (1) above gives, the h om ogeneity of a u d ib ility and prosody of zulu w ords for speech reception thr esho ld testing a + fix p(x) = 1 + e a + fix (2) therefore, p is the proportion correct at any given intensity level. by inserting the intercept and slope into equation (2) we can pre­ dict the percentage correct at any given intensity level. any line drawn tangent to the curve in figure 1, has a slope equal to p*p(x)*[l p(x)]. the steepest slope occurs at p(x) = 0.5. the x value at this slope is usually called the threshold, (intensity re­ quired for 50% intelligibility). a simple equation to calculate threshold is given by x = log ( p / l p ) a p (3) at p = 0.5, a x = ■ p (4) db level exp(a + word a p (x) p) 0.05028 p(x) geza ■1.85 0.228 -5 744 0.08892 0.04788 geza -1.85 0.228 -2.5 162 0.15723 0.08166 geza -1.85 0.228 0 717 0.27803 0.135873 geza -1.85 0.228 2.5 73 0.49164 0.21755 geza -1.85 0.228 5 42 0.86935 0.329599 geza -1.85 0.228 7.5 824 1.53725 0.465057 geza -1.85 0.228 10 752 2.71828 0.605874 geza -1.85 0.228 12.5 183 0.731059 4.80664 geza -1.85 0.228 15 819 0.827784 8.49943 geza -1.85 0.228 17.5 763 0.894731 15.0292 geza -1.85 0.228 20 755 0.937614 26.5757 geza -1.85 0.228 22.5 727 0.963736 46.9930 geza -1.85 0.228 25 632 0.979164 from (4) above we can work out the threshold value example of calculation: p 0.228 estimates of a and (5 are obtained from software package being used (here spss). let us consider the word geza at sound inten­ sity level x = 5. the slope at 50% (p(x) =0.5) or at threshold (x=8.1140) is given by the formula: geza p(v= ea+px e18̂ 22̂ 0 .4 9 1 6 4 4 2 ft , ~ l+ e -u*w [*) ~ 1 .4 9 1 6 4 4 2 i a -1.85 p 0.228 p (x )[l 7?(jc)]/?100 = 0.5(1 0.5)(0.228)100 = 5.7 therefore the level at which audibility has a 50% chance occurs at 8.114 db for the word geza. the plot of p(x) vs db level (x) gives the logistic curve below, g eza thus the logistic model is of the form and p(5) is thus we have the proportion correct at the 5db level. for the lodb level substitute x= 10 in the above. for the 15db level substitute x=15 in the above, and so on. until we arrive at a list like the one below. 1.2 1 0 .8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 -i ■ s e r i e s l calculations for the other words continue in a similar way. the south african journal of communication disorders, vol 56, 2009 75 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) abstract introduction the lexicon: nouns and predicates bantu language acquisition aims and objectives ping assessment tool methods main study ethical considerations results discussion acknowledgements references about the author(s) ramona kunene nicolas department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, south africa saaliha ahmed department of linguistics, university of the witwatersrand, south africa citation kunene nicolas, r., & ahmed, s. (2016). lexical development of noun and predicate comprehension and production in isizulu. south african journal of communication disorders, 63(2), a169. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i2.169 original research lexical development of noun and predicate comprehension and production in isizulu ramona kunene nicolas, saaliha ahmed received: 10 june 2016; accepted: 24 june 2016; published: 28 july 2016 copyright: © 2016. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract this study seeks to investigate the development of noun and predicate comprehension and production in isizulu-speaking children between the ages of 25 and 36 months. it compares lexical comprehension and production in isizulu, using an italian developed and validated vocabulary assessment tool: the picture naming game (ping) developed by bello, giannantoni, pettenati, stefanini and caselli (2012). the ping tool includes four subtests, one each for subnoun comprehension (nc), noun production (np), predicate comprehension (pc), and predicate production (pp). children are shown these lexical items and then asked to show comprehension and produce certain lexical items. after adaptation into the south african context, the adapted version of ping was used to directly assess the lexical development of isizulu with the three main objectives to (1) test the efficiency of the adaptation of a vocabulary tool to measure isizulu comprehension and production development, (2) test previous findings done in many cross-linguistic comparisons that have found that both comprehension and production performance increase with age for a lesser-studied language, and (3) present our findings around the comprehension and production of the linguistic categories of nouns and predicates. an analysis of the results reported in this study show an age effect throughout the entire sample. across all the age groups, the comprehension of the noun and predicate subtests was better performed than the production of noun and predicate subtests. with regard to lexical items, the responses of children showed an influence of various factors, including the late acquisition of items, possible problems with stimuli presented to them, and the possible input received by the children from their home environment. introduction research on the emergence of early lexical comprehension and production is very important for both enhancing our understanding of language acquisition and diagnostic purposes. in the south african context, improving numeracy and literacy skills remains a huge challenge in the public education sector (department of basic education, 2014). language policies favour l1 learning in the foundation years (ages 6–9 years), yet psycholinguistic measures of children’s competencies are not included in either educational or political initiatives. existing literature is very limited regarding the cognitive performance of children who speak a south african indigenous language (also linguistically classified as a bantu language) in their early language acquisition. the other challenge is finding locally appropriate standardised tools that are relatively easy to use in any given language. it is heartening, however, to note that several researchers in speech and language therapy have adapted several international standardised tests to the local context and are starting to include crucial information in the measurement of children’s typical and atypical development. having normative data allows for interventions in delayed or impaired language as well as effective language teaching and learning (kathard et al., 2011; pascoe & smouse, 2012). despite an increase, there are still too few academic studies on the comprehension and production of south african bantu languages. those available include studies focusing on phonological development (gxilishe, 2004; pascoe & smouse, 2012); nominal morphology (gxilishe, 2008); morphosyntactic development of the noun classes (kunene, 1979; tsonope, 1987); the development of the passive construction (bortz, 2013; demuth, 2003); and pragmatic development in late language acquisition (kunene-nicolas, 2015). although these studies have greatly advanced our knowledge of less-studied languages, no one has managed to present comprehensive linguistic research on the developmental aspects of the lexicon inventory of the bantu-speaking child, similar to that regarding children who speak western languages, such as english, french, and italian. researchers tend to focus on particular aspects within linguistic theory (berko gleason & bernstein ratner, 1993, pp. 326–327). to date, the available literature on early language acquisition has confirmed, through the study of many languages from different language typologies, that certain stages of language acquisition are universal (harley, 2014). it is, therefore, widely accepted that all children begin their trajectory towards language through comprehension, while their motoric and cognitive apparatus lags in terms of language production. this study seeks to investigate the development of noun and predicate comprehension (pc) and production in isizulu-speaking children between the ages of 25 and 36 months. the study is part of an international research collaboration that aims to investigate speech and co-speech gesture production and comprehension development in children. it compares lexical comprehension and production of two romance languages, italian and french, and two south african bantu languages, isizulu and sesotho, using an italian developed and validated vocabulary assessment tool. this paper presents the preliminary findings of the lexical development of isizulu speakers. the lexicon: nouns and predicates we use words to communicate about everything related to our physical environment, including events, activities, people, objects, places, relations, properties, and states of being (clark, 1995, p. 1). words stored by language users are drawn from the lexicon that can also be understood as our mental dictionary. bates and goodman (1997) found that, as children transition from the first word stage to sentences and extended discourse, while learning productive control over the basic morphosyntactic structures of their native language, the emergence and elaboration of grammar are highly dependent upon vocabulary size. the lexicon is therefore linked to phonology, comprehension and production, and grammar (gentner, 1982). the child’s lexicon is dependent on the development of meaning construction and categorisation skills (markman, 1991). measuring comprehension and production of nouns and predicates is increasingly used as an important diagnostic and prognostic tool in atypical populations (stefanini, bello, caselli, iverson & volterra, 2009). as such, the macarthur communicative development inventory (cdi) was initially developed to study the relationship between the lexical and grammatical development of english-speaking children (bates, dale & thal, 1995; fenson et al., 1994; fenson, marchman, thal, dale & reznick, 2007) but has since been adapted for use in more than 62 languages. it is well documented that typically developing children are accurately able, by 3.5 years of age, to produce most of the basic morphosyntactic structures of their languages such as relative clauses, the passive construction and other complex forms (bates & goodman, 1997; demuth, 2003). our present study seeks to look at one component of the lexicon: the comprehension and production of the nouns and predicate categories of words in isizulu. nouns and predicates are characterised by differences in their perceptual and cognitive complexity (davidoff & masterson, 1996; gentner, 1982; gentner & boroditsky, 2001), which leads to distinct mental representations (slobin, 2008). in the cross-linguistic study by caselli et al. (1995) the authors highlight the noun-verb sequence in early acquisition as proposed by the ‘whole object constraint’ (markman, 1991). gentner (1982) reported the late appearance of verbs, which are more complex in structure than the underlying semantic structure of nouns. o’grady (1987) pointed out that nouns are used as ‘arguments’ or ‘primaries’ that refer to entities or a class of entities, whereas verbs and adjectives are often used as predicates or ‘secondaries’ (caselli et al., 1995, p. 162). this means that, for a child to produce verbs and adjectives successfully, nominal arguments have to be in place. the acquisition of verbs will therefore be affected by the child’s mastery of nouns. these theoretical arguments, however, have been challenged recently by the appearance of language groups that show children mastering verbs at a faster rate in mandarin (cheng, 1994) and korean (gopnik & choi, 1995). literature does, though, confirm that verbs, adjectives, and function words appear later in early child acquisition (caselli et al., 1995). this study therefore seeks to document the development of the noun and predicate in isizulu. bantu language acquisition bantu languages are typologically similar and share several typical grammatical features. isizulu is a south eastern bantu language of the nguni cluster spoken primarily in south africa (especially the southeastern areas of kwa-zulu natal), but it also has speakers in neighbouring countries. isizulu is highly mutually intelligible with other nguni languages, such as isindebele, isixhosa, and siswati. in 2011, south africans citing isizulu as their home language numbered 11.5 million, or 22.7% of the population, the language that has the highest number of speakers (census, 2011). isizulu is a subject-verb-object (svo) language with a high number (about 15) of noun classes, triggering the agreement of verbs, adjectives, and other elements. in other words, ‘nominal and verbal modifiers follow the noun and verb respectively, and grammatical morphology is prefixed to both nouns and verbs’ (demuth & suzman, 1997, p. 2). the subject can be dropped and it is therefore a pro-drop language as well (gxilishe, villiers & villiers, 2007; kunene, 2010; suzman, 1985, 1991). it has a very rich system of tense and aspect. these are expressed in a variety of simple tenses with optional aspectual affixes, compound tenses allowing composition of many of the simple tenses, and a large number of auxiliary verbs (buell, 2005, p. 6). demuth (2003, pp. 1–4) gives a thorough overview of south african bantu language acquisition studies of siswati, isizulu, isixhosa, setswana, and sesotho. most studies have looked at the noun class prefix and nominal agreement, consonants and clicks, acquisition of word order, relative clauses, and morpho-phonology (for a review on studies of bantu language acquisition, see demuth, 2003; and for a contemporary overview of studies on sa bantu language see gxilishe, 2008; pascoe & smouse, 2012). from existing literature, we know that child speakers of isizulu, sesotho, siswati, and isixhosa have fully acquired the nominal class system by the age of three years. we also know, from a study of isixhosa-speaking children (gxilishe et al., 2007), that the plural agreement is better produced than the singular subject agreement. despite the numerous studies on the isizulu verb (or related languages), we have not come across literature that documents the acquisition of verbs, adjectives, and adverbs or the noun and its morphology. studies on south african bantu languages are definitely increasing, but as yet there has been no study that has looked at simultaneous comprehension and production during lexical development. aims and objectives we focus on the lexical development of comprehension and the production of nouns and predicates from a speech perspective. specifically, this article seeks to explore the lexical development of isizulu using the adapted assessment tool with three main objectives. these are to: test the effectiveness of the adaptation of a vocabulary tool to measure isizulu comprehension and production development. test the universal finding that both comprehension and production performance increase with age for a less-studied language, isizulu. present our findings on the comprehension and production of the linguistic categories of nouns and predicates. ping assessment tool early childhood development research shows a strong interdependence between vocabulary, phonology, and grammar in both typical and atypical populations (marchman & thal, 2005; stoel-gammon, 2011). if children who show delays in their expressive vocabulary repertoires can be identified in time, this could assist in early intervention for children at high risk for language impairment, as shown in the studies by desmarais, sylvestre, meyer, bairati and rouleau (2008) ellis and thal (2008). constructed and validated in italy, the picture naming game (ping) was specifically developed to assess lexicon production and comprehension in children between the ages of 19 and 37 months, involving the consideration of both nouns and predicates, and based on the italian mb-cdi. previous studies have shown that it is extremely relevant to investigate the relationship between vocabulary comprehension and production as well as between nouns and predicates, as these skills and their relationships are indicators of both the level of language development and conceptual organisation. in general, studies using the ping tool with italian children proved that the comprehension subtests were easier than the production subtests, thus allowing for their administration in younger children and resulting in fewer errors. similarly, children found the noun subtests easier than the predicates subtests. therefore, lower variability was found in vocabulary comprehension compared with production, and in nouns compared with predicates for hearing children (bello et al., 2012; rinaldi, caselli, di renzo, gulli & volterra, 2014). the ping tool consists of two sets of colour pictures and contains two tasks; comprehension and production tasks which in turn contain four subtests. the first set has 22 images (20 test pictures and two pre-test pictures) of objects and tools, animals, food and clothing (e.g. a fork, a lion, bananas, gloves) and is used in evaluating the comprehension and production of nouns in the noun comprehension subtest (nc) and the noun production subtest (np), respectively. the second set contains 22 images (20 test pictures and two pre-test pictures) showing actions, location adverbs, and/or adjectives (for example, to push, close by or far away, big or small) and is used to evaluate the comprehension and production of predicates in the pc subtest and the predicate production subtest (pp), respectively. the original ping test for italian children was adapted from the italian mbcdi and the items had different levels of difficulty. it included items that were ‘easy’, ‘moderately easy’, and ‘difficult’, based on the italian normative sample (bello et al., 2012; pettenati, sekine, congestri & volterra, 2012; pettenati, stefanini & volterra, 2009; stefanini et al., 2009; stefanini, recchia & caselli, 2008). in this paper, we report on the adaptation of ping to isizulu. the ping tool has already been successfully adapted to other languages and cultures. for example, a study by pettenati and colleagues provided the first occasion for a cross-cultural comparison of gestures and vocabulary production and comprehension of 22 italian and 22 japanese children between 25 and 37 months of age (pettenati et al., 2012). the ping tool was also used to assess vocabulary production and comprehension in toddlers in a study carried out in australia by hall, rumney, holler and kidd (2013). the australian study focused on a group of 50 typically developing children between 18 and 31 months of age, investigating the interrelationship between play, gesture use, and spoken language development. methods stage 1: translation of the ping lexicon into the target languages translation of the set of nouns (20 target nouns in the comprehension task +20 target nouns in the production task +2 × 2 = 4 lexical items for the pre-tests) and the set of predicates (20 target verbs and/or adverbs and/or adjectives in the comprehension task +20 target verbs and/or adverbs and/or adjectives in the production task +2 × 2 = 4 lexical items for the pre-tests) was carried out in isizulu by the researcher, who is a native speaker of isizulu and a linguist, together with two isizulu-speaking research assistants, who are also linguists. the translation was further tested in a pilot study of native zulu adults for validation (see stage 2). of particular interest in the international collaborative comparative study is the different language typology of romance languages that are analytic and bantu languages that are agglutinative. in the initial adaptation, a conscious decision was made to adapt the protocol questions as closely as possible to the original italian version, that is, questions were to be ‘neutral’ so as not to give a clue to the answer. for example, in the italian version, the question would be translated to ‘show me running’, which does not provide any clue to the participant for the comprehension task and, therefore, the speaker cannot get a clue on the referent. a participant could choose any item he or she deemed fit. however, because of the noun class system, agreement concords, and morphosyntax structure of bantu languages, the question must have the relevant noun class and subject concord, which may give a clue to the item. for instance, in isizulu, a semantic translation for the above example would read ngikhombise ogijimayo (show me the person that is running): this would not allow a speaker to select a different card in the task because the question requires the speaker to select a card with a person who is doing something, for example, running. stage 2: pilot study with adults twenty two adults (11 males and 11 females) participated in the zulu adult pilot study. participants were drawn from the pre-dominant isizulu speakers of kwa-zulu natal, the south-east region of south africa. participants were university students from various communities in the kwa-zulu natal area, 60% of whom were from the empangeni area. the other 40% were from surrounding areas: pongola, harrismith, durban, eshowe, and pietermaritzburg. applying a neutral questioning style did not work. participants would reformulate the question or stop the interviewer to ask for more clarity. if the questions were amended, participants answered with no difficulty. the inclusion of the class prefixes did not affect the results but rather assisted the participant in understanding what was requested of him or her. this was indicated by the fact that once the correct class prefixes were used, the participants would indicate that they did not recognise an item, or they would give an answer if they did. with the neutral questions, the participant would simply halt the interview, seek clarification, and personally supply the class prefixes. when the interviewer asked why the participant reformulated the utterances, all participants stated that the ‘neutral’ utterances were grammatically correct, but ambiguous and confusing. it is interesting to note that all 22 participants corrected the utterances. agreement between participants was 95% for the comprehension subset and 86% for the production subset. four items under the np subset produced either no responses or ‘i do not know’ answers, referring specifically to bidet, radiator, penguin, and seal. under the pp, two predicate task words produced a low frequency of correct target words (spinning, heavy, far apart). stage 3: modification of the material from the adult pilot results, it became clear that the isizulu version of ping needed further adaptation before the pilot study with children was initiated. the adults seldom produced words in isizulu for ‘seal’ and ‘penguin’ and so these items were changed to ‘snail’ and ‘crocodile’, respectively. as both ‘radiator’ and ‘bidet’ are foreign cultural objects, these two items were replaced by ‘heater’ and ‘toilet’. some pictures were specifically cultural, such as the picture of the ‘roof’, which was a european type of roof, but in order to allow a systematic comparison with other languages in the four-language project, some items were retained for future adaptation. stage 4: pilot study with children after the changes to the above-mentioned picture items, a pilot study was conducted with 15 zulu children. the group included five children aged 25 months (±1 month), five children aged 30 months (±1 month), and five children aged 36 months (±1 month). this was done in order to test the corresponding adaptation of ping on a small sample in case further adaptation was needed before going onto the main study. participants were drawn from kwa-zulu natal, the same area where the adult pilot study was conducted. data was collected from the urban townships of empangeni and ngwelezane on the northern coast of kwa-zulu natal. the principals and caregivers were very helpful in providing the researchers with the children’s clinic cards. these vaccination cards aided the researchers in selecting participants for the appropriate age cohorts. the files provided by the teachers also gave the researchers additional information, including, for example, whether a child had been born prematurely or had a learning disability and needed to be excluded from the selected participants. we worked with nine crèches to ensure that our three age groups were exact. many crèches could not form part of our participant sample because those children were bilingual and would alternate between naming items in english and isizulu. we finally chose two schools from the city centre of empangeni and four schools in the ngwelezane township. administration of the ping tool began with a familiarisation phase that involved playing various card games, counting games, and naming games with the children. researchers also played with the children in the school playgrounds on the swings and slides. once the researchers felt that the children were comfortable enough, they asked the children to play a game with them in front of the camera. main study the main study for the isizulu data was collected from soweto in the gauteng province. the move from kzn was purely logistical as the researchers were all gauteng based. monolingual isizulu-speaking children were chosen with the help of their caregivers. children’s vaccination cards were examined to exclude premature babies or those with any recorded pathologies. all crèches require clinic or vaccination cards in order to enrol the child. the caregivers also assisted in selecting children, who they said showed no language delays in comparison to their peers. all selected children had parental consent (see the ethics section). forty-nine children from four neighbouring crèches participated in the study. nine children were excluded from the data sample for various reasons: some children did not complete the two tasks, some children were bilingual and code-switched regularly, some children spoke too softly for the camcorder to record sound, and one child was sleepy and had to go for a nap. for this study, 36 participants were chosen with 12 per age cohort in consideration of gender balance. there were a total of 19 females and 17 males across the different age cohorts (table 1). table 1: zulu participants age groups. procedure the procedure of this study followed those of previous studies (bello et al., 2012; pettenati et al., 2009, 2012; stefanini et al., 2008, 2009). the tool began with the comprehension task picture, followed immediately by the production-eliciting picture of the noun sets. there was a short break after the noun items and then the predicate items would be elicited, again starting with the comprehension task picture, followed by the production-eliciting picture of that set. the following section details the procedure that was followed in our study. after the familiarisation period, during which we played different card games with the children, all children were tested individually at their schools. three sets of pictures per set were presented to each child on a small table. the first part of the task was comprehension, in which a child was asked ‘where is the cat? show me the cat’ for a noun comprehension item or ‘what is this child doing? what is this one doing?’ for the pc item. the second part of the each subset was production, in which the child was asked, ‘what is this?’ for the np subset or ‘what is he doing?’ for the pp subset. the third card was a distractor to eliminate the choice by luck. a total of 22 cards were presented for the noun subtest, and another 22 cards were presented for the predicate subtest. the first two sets were pre-test cards to ensure that the child understood what was expected. the data was based on the remaining 20-card set per subtest. for the comprehension task, only one prompt was considered. for the production task, if the child struggled with producing the correct item, a second prompt was used. all elicitations were filmed for later data coding and analysis. two research assistants and the researcher of this study collected the data. all researchers are first-language, native speakers of isizulu, linguists with fieldwork experience in the collection of data from children. coding and transcription of the data the coding system was adapted from previous studies (bello et al., 2012 pettenati et al., 2012). all the children’s responses were coded later from the video data with an annotation system that was designed for the purpose of coding for gestures as well as for wordings from the child and the experimenter. all tasks administered to the children were coded on elan, a linguistic annotation tool created by the max planck institute (elan, n.d.; wittenburg, brugman, russel, klassmann & sloetjies, 2006). for the comprehension subtests (nc and pc), if the child indicated (either by pointing, showing, or verbalising) the photograph corresponding to the item indicated by the adult researcher, the answer was considered correct. if the child selected the no target photograph or did not respond at all, the response was coded as incorrect or no response, respectively. similarly, in the production subtests, if the child produced the target lexical item, their response was coded as correct. if the child produced a non-target item or did not respond at all, their response was coded as incorrect or no response, respectively. for some photographs, more than one answer was accepted as correct; for instance, for the ‘diaper’ item, some children called it ipampers referring to the brand of disposable nappies or ‘ikhimbi’ also referring to a brand of disposable nappies. for the production subtest, children were prompted twice if their initial response was incorrect. if a correct response was produced after the second prompt, the answer was considered to be correct. synonymous items were considered to be correct synonyms, for instance ilorri (a lorry) and itruck (a truck) for the truck item. responses that had a semantic relationship to the item depicted were coded as nts, no target, but semantically correct to measure if the concept was in place even though the production was not successful. incorrect responses occurred where the production was not the target response nor semantically related, for instance, isidudu (motorbike) for ‘truck’. validation and reliability three trained native isizulu-speaking research assistants and the researcher independently coded the verbal transcription, that is, orthographical transcription directly from the film footage. two different research assistants, who are also trained linguists, coded the classification of the speech responses as well as those of gesture. disagreements were resolved through discussion. after the annotation phase was completed, all data was exported to excel for an ultimate verification (internal consistency on the coding) and statistical analysis. ethical considerations ethical considerations guided the pilot study as well as the main study. all children who participated were recruited on a voluntary basis after their caregivers signed an informed consent form, which was provided in their language, and after they themselves agreed to participate at the start of the task (‘nouns’ or ‘predicates’). parents or members of the crèche were welcomed in the room during the administration of the tool. the tasks were interrupted or ended if a child verbalised a desire to stop, or expressed discomfort by crying and/or withdrawing. children’s identities were kept confidential, and data obtained from this project were not disclosed to any third party. the wits hrec non-medical ethics committee granted ethical clearance for the study (protocol number h13/08/43). results children’s responses were analysed according to the coding criteria listed in the method section. for our first objective, we analysed the comprehension and production tasks across the three age groups to test whether the ping assessment tool was effective in detecting the development of comprehension and production in isizulu. our second objective overlapped our first, and so our first finding addresses both of our objectives. for the comprehension and production tasks, an analysis of variance, anova, was run with the age group as the independent variable. the correct answers for the comprehension and production task items are illustrated in table 2. table 2: comprehension and production task scores per age group. a significant difference across the age groups emerged at (f[2.69] = 3.143, p < 0.05 for the comprehension task. post-hoc bonferroni analysis showed that the effect of age was significant at 0.05 among all age groups throughout the entire sample, but was not significant between the 25-month-old and the 30-month-old groups. similarly, for the production task, there was a significant age effect across the whole sample at (f[2.69] = 6.567, p < 0.05. post-hoc bonferroni analysis showed that the difference was not significant between the 25-month-old and the 30-month-old groups. however the 36-month group performed significantly better than the two younger groups at p < 0.003 to the 25-month group and p < 0.035 to the 30-month group. lexical item composition in order to test the comprehension and production tasks per lexical categories, we looked at the performance of the nouns and predicates across the age groups. we performed an anova between groups per lexical subset. the comprehension of the noun and predicate subtests was better performed than the production of noun and predicate subtests across the age groups. in table 3 and table 4, zulu children performed better at labelling the correct items for nouns (f[2,33] = 3.70, p < 0.04) than labelling the correct items for predicates (f[2,33] = 0.94, p < 0.40) with post-hoc test bonferroni confirming that there was a significant difference between the 36-month group and the 25-month group at p < 0.03. there was no significant difference between the 25-month and the 30-month groups, nor between the 30-month and the 36-month groups for the noun subtest. for the pc subtest, post-hoc bonferroni test confirmed that age had no significant effect, although there was a developmental trend between the different age groups. table 3: lexical comprehension noun and predicate subtest means (sd) per age group. table 4: lexical production noun and predicate subtest means (sd) per age group. for the production of nouns and predicates, we noted a similar pattern in that children performed better at labelling the correct items in the np subtest (f[2,33] = 7.4, p < 0.002) than in the pp subtest (f(2,33) = 0.96, p < 0.393). post-hoc bonferroni tests confirmed that there was a significant difference between the 36-month group with both the 25-month group and 30-month group, at p < 0.003 and p < 0.02, respectively. for the pp subtest, the post-hoc bonferroni test confirmed that age had no significant effect across the three age groups. the 36-month children had an equal chance of correctly labelling pp items as the children in the 25-month and 30-month groups, despite the developmental trend we observe in table 3 and table 4. percentage of correct responses per item for our third objective, we present our findings on the performance of the lexical items. in order to have a better understanding of the performance on the comprehension and production task, as well as noun and predicate subtests, we ranked the items in terms of correctness according to the total sample shown in table 5. table 5: percentage of correct answers for comprehension and production provided by (n = 36) children. comprehension task under the noun comprehension subtest, three items were perceived correctly by all 36 children; ‘doll’, ‘hat’, and ‘boots’ had a 100% response rate across all ages. five items had less than 50% success; ‘mountain’, ‘snail’, ‘elephant’, ‘bib’, and ‘hammer’, meaning that fewer than 18 children across the three age groups found these items ‘difficult’. the photograph of the mountain showed a rising mountain of the european alps, a type of geographical feature that is not commonly seen in south africa. it was interesting to note that the children had difficulty identifying ‘snail’ and ‘elephant’, but managed easily to identify the domestic animals such as ‘cow’. the ‘bib’ and ‘hammer’ were also not easily identified: the children gave answers that focused more on the baby who was wearing the ‘bib’ and did not respond at all to the ‘hammer’ or said, angazi (i do not know). under the pc subset, there were seven items that had less than 50% success; ‘walk’, ‘behind’, ‘close’, ‘full’, ‘outside’, ‘to embrace’, and ‘short’. some of the responses for ‘walk’ were uhamba kuphi (walking where?), as the picture showed a young boy walking along the passage of a house. the smaller children would either focus on the clothes the child was wearing or on the distractor card, which showed a young boy playing with toys. this performance did not show a developmental trend, which meant the 36-month-old children also had a similar chance of not perceiving or identifying ‘the walking’ from this picture. the item ‘embrace’ also produced some interesting comments across the age groups, similar to ‘kiss’ found in the pp subtest. the older the children, the more they avoided discussing intimacy with responses like bayaganga (they are being naughty) or they would simply avoid looking at the picture and focus on other pictures. the adjectives ‘short’, ‘full’, ‘outside’, and ‘behind’ were very difficult items that showed a distinct developmental pattern with responses that were semantically related, so for ‘short’, a child would say yincane (it is small) referring to a short pencil in the picture, and yet the prompt was for them to point to the object, which does not necessitate a verbal response. production task overall, production items scored lower than comprehension items, as seen in table 5. under the production subtests, for the np, the items ‘comb’ and ‘socks’ had the highest response rate at 92%, with children from all age groups correctly labelling these items. ten items had a success rate of less than 50%; the items were: ‘gloves’, ‘diaper’, ‘picture’, ‘truck’, ‘book’, ‘beach’, ‘lion’, ‘crocodile’, ‘roof’, and ‘flags’. the 36-month-old group was more familiar with the leather gloves depicted in the picture with most children saying izandla/into yezandla zikamama (hands or something for my mum’s hands). glove sizes in south african shops start for children who are about 4 to 5 years of age, and mittens are not commonly found. the older children labelled the item ‘diaper’ correctly, compared with the young children who tended to call it ipenti (a panty). the ‘picture’ item was also more familiar to the 36-month-old group, even though most children across the age groups identified it as a tv, because the picture looked like a flat screen tv. the picture depicted a lone beach with a blue sky, the ocean, and a strip of sand. the picture also garnered ambiguous responses from the adults in the pilot study. the item ‘truck’ produced the semantically related ‘a car or a bus or a big car’. this item did not display any developmental trajectory, as 36-month-old children were equally likely to produce ‘car’ rather than the target word itruck or ilori. the ‘book’ item mostly produced ibhayibheli (a bible) across age groups as well. the ‘beach’ item produced equally random responses, with children either focusing on the water in the photo or on the sky. the wild animals ‘lion’ and ‘crocodile’ produced very interesting responses such as ikgokgo (a monster) or some onomatopoeia sounds illustrating that ‘this is a scary thing that will eat me’. a few children avoided even looking at the picture or quickly pushed the card to the researcher. the ‘roof’ item depicted a european type roof and a portion of a house with trees surrounding it. most responses were either indlu (house) or isihlahla (a tree). this picture did not reveal an age pattern because the responses were random. the item ‘flags’ was extremely difficult, with no child giving a correct response. most responses were iafrica (africa) or yiduku (head scarf), and these semantically related responses increased with age. in the pp subtest, the following seven items were difficult for the children: ‘to turn’, ‘empty’, ‘in front of’, ‘heavy’, ‘far’, ‘inside’, and ‘long’. the item for ‘to turn’ depicted a group of children on a merry-go-round. children’s responses included bayadlala/bahleli (they are playing or they are sitting). this response did not reveal a developmental trajectory because the responses were random across all age groups. for the adjectives and adverbs ‘empty’, ‘in front of’, ‘far’, ‘inside’, and ‘long’, the responses did not show correct labelling across all ages. these items showed a developmental pattern when considered in the light of that the responses showed that the children acquired this concept with age. interestingly enough, five of these items were in the same set at the pc subtest, which shows consistency in the production of these categories. all the children had difficulty perceiving the item ‘heavy’. this item depicted a young child carrying a brown torn box while grimacing. children’s responses focused on the child or the box being torn. discussion our study begins with an italian picture naming assessment tool being adapted to a bantu language, isizulu. the tool is designed to directly observe the lexical composition of vocabulary in two related tasks; comprehension and production. to our knowledge, most studies on isizulu or related bantu languages have either directly observed either comprehension or production, but never both at the same time. several pilot studies in both adult and children populations enabled us to alter obvious elements of cultural bias. although some other problematic items remained, we decided to preserve our initial goal by keeping as many items as necessary for our systematic comparison with two romance languages, italian and french, as well as another south african bantu language, sesotho. certain items such as the picture of a ‘roof’ did not depict a ‘roof’ as seen by many south african children. some items such as ‘to turn’, which depicted a merry-go-round produced unexpected results in that one would expect most children to have been exposed to a merry-go-round as these are commonly found in parks, but instead the children focused more on the people in the picture than on what they were doing. despite the cultural differences that may have stemmed from the images of our stimulus, we note that our findings confirm what has been long documented in literature: that comprehension comes before production. in a related cross-linguistic comparison, japanese children showed a lower lexical production compared with the italian children (pettenati et al., 2012) which resulted in the authors finding that cultural factors could influence the design of the test which was originally for italian children. in terms of development, our results show how age affects both comprehension and production: 36-month-old children performed better than 30-month-old children who in turn performed better than 25-month-old children. our statistical analysis did not discern a significant difference between the 25-month-old group and the 30-month-old group for comprehension, but it detected a significant difference between the 36-month to the 25-month and 30-month groups in terms of production. this was not surprising as, in the first study by bello et al. (2012), it was found that noun comprehension increases between 19 and 30 month, followed by a plateau. the zulu children also showed this plateau, which meant there was little difference between the 25-month and 30-month groups. the pc showed a similar trend, even though the scores were lower than those of the noun comprehension. it is, however, very interesting to note that there was no significant difference in pc among the groups, which may mean that predicates in this task, with the exception of adverbs and adjectives, may be mastered earlier. gxilishe et al. (2007) found that isixhosa-speaking children between 24 and 30 months correctly employed subject agreement markers on different verb roots. in terms of production, our findings showed an overall effect of age, which meant that production does indeed lag behind comprehension. moreover, the larger number of culturally foreign pictures in the noun comprehension subtest suggests that it may be necessary to further adapt the assessment tool to more effectively evaluate the isizulu lexicon. children used a semantic description strategy to try and explain unfamiliar items, which shows that though they may have the concept, the lexical item is not ‘concrete’ enough for them to relate to their physical environment. if perception is difficult, it is harder to retrieve the semantic representation from the lexicon and, as such, retrieval will be impaired (harley, 2014). the production of the noun category showed an age effect but the predicate category did not show a similar difference. the higher number of adverbs and adjectives in the pp subtest was difficult for all the age groups, which could explain the lack of a significant difference. alternatively, if verbs are acquired earlier, it shows that the children have reached a plateau phase between the ages of 24 and 36 months. further research on the vocabulary spurt in isizulu would shed more light on this issue. limitations the lack of a child inventory like the macarthur bates cdi for isizulu and related languages is a disadvantage. we have no idea which items are acquired first, nor do we have the exact age of the vocabulary spurt in isizulu. future analysis should look at the gender effect. although gender-related data is available in this study, it has not yet been analysed. it would be interesting to see whether girls have an advantage over boys, as has frequently been reported in western languages. conclusion developing normative lexical data on zulu-speaking children or those speaking other related bantu languages is important for research for both acquisition and clinical purposes. finding a standardised assessment tool that can be used for south african bantu languages is an ongoing challenge. the ping assessment tool has proved to be robust and effective in directly assessing a child’s lexical vocabulary. for further research into isizulu, the tool would need further adaptation by replacing some images with items of local content. the literature states that children begin with language comprehension and, when the motoric and cognitive apparatus develops, language production follows suit. this is a universal linguistic parameter. children start with objects and events around their immediate environment and quickly learn people’s names, concrete objects around them, and familiar routines coming from their home environment. the first words will therefore largely depend on this input, and cultural as well as linguistic constraints may affect this development. this study shows that as children get older, comprehension and production improve. it would therefore be very important for researchers or speech therapists to factor input into their intervention therapies. children will understand and talk more about what they know and what surrounds them. some linguistic phenomena like adjectives and adverbs are complex and not yet acquired by the zulu child at 36 months. acknowledgements competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions r.k.n. was the project leader and was responsible for the experiment, data collection and data management. s.a. performed most of the coding, prepared the samples, and contributed to the conceptualising and analysing of the data samples. a qualitative analysis was done by both authors. references bates, e., dale, p.s., & thal, d. (1995). individual differences and their implications for theories of language development. in paul fletcher & brian macwhinney (eds.), the handbook of child language (pp. 96–151). oxford: basil blackwell. bates, e., & goodman, j. c. (1997). on the inseparability of grammar and the lexicon: evidence from acquisition, aphasia and real-time processing. language and cognitive processes, 12(5–6), 507–584. bello, a., giannantoni, p., pettenati, p., stefanini, s., & caselli, m.c. (2012). assessing lexicon: validation and developmental data of the picture naming game (ping), a new picture naming task for toddlers. international journal of language & communication disorders, 47(5), 589–602. gleason, j. berko & ratner, n. bernstein. (1993). language development in children. in j. berko gleason & n. bernstein ratner (eds.), psycholinguistics (pp. 301–350). fort worth: harcourt, brace. bortz, m.a. (2013). the acquisition of the passive by sestwana-speaking preschoolers. unpublished doctoral dissertation, the university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg buell, l.c. (2005). issues in zulu verbal morphosyntax. doctoral dissertation university of california los angeles: proquest llc. caselli, m.c., bates, e., casadio, p., fenson, j., fenson, l., sanderl, l., et al., (1995). a cross-linguistic study of early lexical development. cognitive development, 10(2), 159–199. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0885-2014(95)90008-x cheng, s.w. (1994). beginning words of three children acquiring mandarin chinese. taiwan: manuscript, national chung cheng university. clark, e.v. (1995). language acquisition: the lexicon and syntax. in j.l. miller & p.d. eimas (eds.), speech language and communication. handbook of perception and cognition, vol. 11 (pp. 303–337). san diego, ca: academic press. davidoff, j., & masterson, j. (1996). the development of picture naming: differences between verbs and nouns. journal of neurolinguistics, 9(2), 69–83. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/0911-6044(96)00004-8 demuth, k. (2003). the acquisition of bantu languages. in d. nurse & g. philipson (eds.), the bantu languages (pp. 209–222). surrey, england: curzon press. demuth, k., & suzman, s. (1997). language impairment in zulu. in e.m huges & a. green (eds.), proceedings of the 21st annual boston university conference on language development, vol. 1 (pp. 124–135). somerville, ma: cascadilla press. desmarais, c., sylvestre, a., meyer, f., bairati, i., & rouleau, n. (2008). systematic review of the literature on characteristics of late-talking toddlers. international journal of language & communication disorders, 43(4), 361–389. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/13682820701546854 elan. (n.d.). max planck institute for psycholinguistics, the language archive, nijmegen, the netherlands. retrieved april 11, 2016, from http://tla.mpi.nl/tools/tla-tools/elan/ ellis, e.m., & thal, d.j. (2008). early language delay and risk for language impairment. sig 1 perspectives on language learning and education, 15(3), 93–100. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/lle15.3.93 fenson, l., dale, p., reznick, j.s., thal, d., bates, e., hartung, j., pethick, s., & reilly, j.s. (1994). the macarthur-bates communicative development inventories: user’s guide and technical manual. san diego, ca: singular press. fenson, l., marchman, v.a., thal, d.j., dale, p.s., & reznick, j.s. (2007). macarthur-bates communicative development inventories: user’s guide and technical manual brookes. baltimore: paul h brookes. gentner, d. (1982). why nouns are learned before verbs: linguistic relativity versus natural partitioning. in s. kuczaj (ed.), language development: vol. 2. language, thought and culture (pp. 301–334). hillsdale, nj: erlbaum. gentner, d., & boroditsky, l. (2001). individuation, relativity, and early word learning. in m. bowerman & s.c. levinson (eds.), language acquisition and conceptual development (pp. 215–256). new york: cambridge university press. gopnik, a., & choi, s. (1995). names, relational words, and cognitive development in englishand korean speakers: nouns are not always learned before verbs. in m. tomasello & w. merriman (eds.), beyond names for things: young children’s acquisition of verbs (pp. 63–80). hillsdale, nj: lawrence erlbaum. gxilishe, s. (2004). the acquisition of clicks by xhosa speaking children. per linguam, 20(2), 1–12. gxilishe, s. (2008). african languages, linguistics, child speech and speech pathology – the connection. per linguam, 24(2), 75–87. gxilishe, s., de villiers, p., & de villiers, j. (2007). the acquisition of subject agreement in xhosa. in a. belikova, l. meroni, & m. umeda (eds.), proceedings of the conference on generative approaches to language acquisition (galana), vol. 2 (pp. 114–123). north america somerville, ma: cascadilla press. hall, s., rumney, l., holler, j., & kidd, e. (2013). associations among play, gesture and early spoken language acquisition. first language, 33(3), 294–312. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0142723713487618 harley, t.a. (2014). the psychology of language: from data to theory. hove and new york: psychology press. kathard, h., ramma, l., pascoe, m., jordaan, h., moonsamy, s., wium, a.m., et al., (2011). how can speech-language therapists and audiologists enhance language and literacy outcomes in south africa? (and why we urgently need to). south african journal of communication disorders, 58(2), 59. kunene, e.c.l. (1979). the acquisition of siswati as a first language: a morphological study with special reference to noun prefixes, noun classes and some agreement markers. unpublished doctoral dissertation, university of california, los angeles, ca. kunene, r.n. (2010). a comparative study of the development of multimodal narratives in french and zulu children and adults. unpublished doctoral dissertation, université stendhal, grenoble. kunene-nicolas, r. (2015). zulu oral narrative development from a speech and gesture perspective. per linguam, 31(3), 1–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.5785/31-3-560 marchman, v., & thal, d. (2005). word and grammar. in m. tomasello & d.i. slobin (eds.), beyond nature–nurture: essays in honor of elizabeth bates (pp. 139–164). mahwah, nj: erlbaum lawrence. markman, e.m. (1991). categorization and naming in children: problems of induction. mit press. department of basic education. (2014). report on the annual national assessments of 2014. retrieved january 16, 2016, from http://www.saqa.org.za/docs/rep_annual/2014/report%20on%20the%20ana%20of%202014.pdf o’grady, w. (1987). principles of grammar & learning. university of chicago press. pascoe, m., & smouse, m. (2012). masithethe: speech and language development and difficulties in isixhosa. samj: south african medical journal, 102(6), 469–471. pettenati, p., sekine, k., congestrì, e., & volterra, v. (2012). a comparative study on representational gestures in italian and japanese children. journal of nonverbal behavior, 36(2), 149–164. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s10919-011-0127-0 pettenati, p., stefanini, s., & volterra, v. (2009). motoric characteristics of representational gestures produced by young children in a naming task. journal of child language, 37(4), 887–911. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000909990092 rinaldi, p., caselli, m. c., di renzo, a., gulli, t., & volterra, v. (2014). sign vocabulary in deaf toddlers exposed to sign language since birth. journal of deaf studies and deaf education, march. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/deafed/enu007 slobin, d. i. (2008). relations between paths of motion and paths of vision: a crosslinguistic and developmental exploration. in v. m. gathercole (ed.). routes to language: studies in honor of melissa bowerman (pp. 197-221). mahwah, nj: lawrence erlbaum associates. statistics south africa. (2011). census. retrieved february 12, 2016 from http://www.statssa.gov.za/?page_id=3839 stefanini, s., bello, a., caselli, m.c., iverson, j.m., & volterra, v. (2009). co-speech gestures in a naming task: developmental data. language and cognitive processes, 24(2), 168–189. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/01690960802187755 stefanini, s., recchia, m., & caselli, m.c. (2008). the relationship between spontaneous gesture production and spoken lexical ability in children with down syndrome in a naming task. gesture, 8(2), 197–218. http://dx.doi.org/10.1075/gest.8.2.05ste stoel-gammon, c. (2011). relationships between lexical and phonological development in young children. journal of child language, 38(1), 1–34. http://dx.doi.org/10.1017/s0305000910000425 suzman, s. (1991). language acquisition in zulu. unpublished doctoral dissertation, the university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. suzman, s.m. (1985). learning the passive in zulu. papers and reports on child language development, 24, 131–137. tsonope, j. (1987). the acquisition of setswana noun class and agreement morphology-with special reference to demonstratives and possessives. doctoral dissertation, state university of new york, buffalo. wittenburg, p., brugman, h., russel, a., klassmann, a., & sloetjes, h. (2006). elan: a professional framework for multimodality research. in proceedings of lrec 2006, fifth international conference on language resources and evaluation. nijmengen, netherlands: max planck institute for psychoolinguistics, the language archive. retrieved april 11, 2016. http://tla.mpi.nl/tools/tla-tools/elan/ ι ο j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y o c t o b e r the role of speech audiometry in practical otology by d. r. haynes, m.b., b.ch. (rand), d.l.o. (r.c.p. & s.) registrar, johannesburg general hospital. it is only recently that a method for measuring hearing in terms of speech discrimination has p a s s e d from the field of laboratory research to the practical field within easy reach of the practising otologist. we may well ask how large a part machines are entitled to play in the practice of medicine and i have heard audiometry condemned by one of the eminent american otologists on the grounds that it does not make a diagnosis and it is not necessary in the treatment of ear disease. in both respects i think he is wrong, but his being wrong is not quite a s obvious a s his being right appears to be. the majority of cases of meniere's disease may be diagnosed on the history alone. nobody, however, would think of making a diagnosis of the disease without carrying out a complete clinical examination of the patient, including tuning fork tests. nobody should diagnose the condition without carrying out tests of labyrinthine function. and, when all this has been done, there will remain a small number of c a s e s in which audiometry gives the final clue to diagnosis when the other tests have failed. the course of disease is of tremendous importance in its treatment and in assessing the progress of hearing or of deterioration of hearing, audiometry plays perhaps an even greater part than it does in the diagnosis. modern neuro-otology, a s this branch of surgery is called by cawthorne, would scarcely be justified if it were not' for our ability to measure and record patients' hearing. speech audiometry by itself is not a complete measurement of hearing ability and in the first place pure-tone audiometry should be employed. there are a great many advanta g e s to the use of speech audiometry but unfortunately it cannot provide all the answers usually required in assessing hearing. the disadvantages of speech audiometry are for the most part of a technical nature. it is easy, for instance, to obtain a perfectly satisfactory pure-tone audiogram from an unintelligent subject. in fact, my experience has been that the unintelligent subject usually gives a more consistently reproducible audiogram than the intelligent, introspective type of person. if standard, comparable results are to be obtained in speech audiometry, the subjects must be reasonably intelligent and, to a certain extent, educated. in this regard, i must mention the speech audiograms that were taken of a professor of l a n g u a g e s at one of the american universities. his mother tongue was german but he taught in english and french and spoke all three l a n g u a g e s fluently. in spite of this the speech audiograms in the three l a n g u a g e s showed a very much better discrimination for the german word lists than for either of the other two. here w a s a highly educated, intelligent man whose audiograms for the extra languages might have been interpreted a s showing a hearing loss when there was none in fact. in exactly the same way, differences in dialect may give results in speech audiometry for uneducated subjects which are not commensurate with the pure-tone audiograms. to overcome these difficulties to a certain sxtent, the monitored live voice may be employed instead of calibrated records but this does not altogether obviate the undersirable features of the test. it becomes obvious now that assessment of hearing must be done with pure-tone and speech audiograms side by side. a good exercise is to forecast the speech audiogram after examining the amount of residual hearing for the accepted speech frequencies on the pure-tone audiogram. this can usually be done with a fair degree of accuracy in the c a s e of conductive deafness. with many cases of perceptive deafness, however, the forecast is found to be grossly inaccurate and this applies particularly in those cases exhibiting the phenomenon of recruitment. here it is of interest to mention that an intelligent adult can obtain a high discrimination score even if he has no hearing for frequencies above 500 c.p.s. using the master hearing aid such a s that used for research in hearing aids by the british medical research council, it is possible to cut out all frequencies above 500 c.p.s. and yet obtain good speech audiograms, even with p.b. word lists. in practice, speech audiograms can be done quickly, especially when a rough assessment r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y of hearing for speech is made according to the pure-tone audiogram. in c a s e s of conductive deafness, the curve will be parallel to the normal curve. in perceptive deafness, the curve flattens out a s the percentage of discrimination increases and it may never reach the 100% level. when the phenomenon of recruitment is present, the percentage discrimination actually falls with the increase of intensity above a certain level and it is therefore essential in cases of recruitment to complete the audiogram in order to get the whole picture. conductive deafness. from the patient's point of view, the most important aspect of hearing is his hearing for speech. with conductive deafness, the speech audiogram follows a regular pattern and can be a s s e s s e d with a fair degree of accuracy from the pure-tone audiogram. our experience with fenestration surgery has led us to believe that if we wish accurately to determine the improvement or otherwise of hearing following surgery we must have speech audiograms a s well a s pure tone audiograms. the best illustration of this is a feature that i have noticed repeatedly with fenestrations, regardless of how expert the surgeon might be. in attempting to a s s e s s the suitability of c a s e s for fenestration, the otologist usually regards the patient with a rising curve in the high frequencies on pure tone audiograms a s having the better prospects of success than the patient with a straight curve or with a curve dropping in the high frequencies. yet there is a very large proportion of fenestrated cases whose post-operative puretone audiograms show a very successful rise in the hearing over the speech frequencies but a marked! drop in the high frequencies. in other words, the post-operative curve is reversed. if an a g g r e g a t e of all frequency changes were to be made in these cases, the sum would probably b e zero. the speech audiograms give a completely different picture in these successful fenestrations and the curve is merely moved bodily to the left which is much more in keeping with the general clinical expectations. this fact is then a very important one in making preand post-operative speech audiograms an essential part of fenestration surgery. perceptive deafness. the attempt to match hearing aids to puretone audiograms has been a failure. this may b e possible in cases of conductive deafness but with perceptive deafness trial and error becomes a necessity for fitting a hearing aid. once again, the speech audiogram is in itself a very much better indication of suitability for hearing aid than is the pure-tone audiogram by itself. if recruitment is shown to be present on a speech audiogram, then it is at once obvious that pure amplification of sound over all frequencies will not benefit the patient. these are the c a s e s in which there is an indication for'modification of the hearing aid by means of automatic loudness control. the speech audiometer in a c a s e of this sort acts a s a master hearing aid and if the speech audiogram shows a marked degree of recruitment it may be assumed that the patient will not benefit from the use of a hearing aid. recruitment. speech audiograms in themselves may give a very much better indication of the degree of recruitment present than the more tedious and difficult tests of loudness balance. they have the further a d v a n t a g e of indicating recruitment when both ears are affected, whereas the pure-tone audiograms are of use only when one ear is affected and the other ear normal. if loudness balance tests are to be reliable, they must be carried out over several frequencies and the time necessary to do this is considerable. furthermore, the patient is easily fatigued and usually two or three attempts are necessary to complete the test. the only condition in which recruitment regularly occurs is meniere's disease and for this reason speech audiometry is an essential part of the investigation of this condition. in spite of the fact that most modern audiometers are fitted with loudness balance attachments, the only satisfactory way of carrying out accurate tests for loudness balance is by the use of two separate, equally calibrated or corrected audiometers or their equivalent. the tests should be done for at least three frequencies and because of this the test is scarcely to be regarded a s of great value to the practising otologist. speech audiometry is readily available nowadays to any otologist and speech audiograms are easily done so that here is a practical indication of recruitment that has a tremendous diagnostic value. occasionally, a mild degree of recruitment is present in cases of presbyacusis and it may, of course, occur immediately after trauma to r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y o c t o b e r the labyrinth. in the latter condition, the recruitment tends to disappear with the p a s s a g e of time after the trauma. children. speech audiometry has a special place in the assessment of hearing in children. puretone audiometry is notoriously inaccurate in children and it is extremely difficult to reproduce accurate audiograms. speech audiograms, however, may be very successfully employed in children of a v e r a g e intelligence over the a g e of four years. if records are made with a carrier phrase, such a s "you will now say . . .," there is very little difficulty in obtaining accurate audiograms which maintain the interest of the child. these audiograms are used in children up to the a g e of 10 or 12 years and in the occasional c a s e s of meniere's disease encountered in this a g e group, the above remarks on the assessment of recruitment apply even more strongly. social adequacy index. the social adequacy index is designed to give an indication of the intelligibility oi speech for a particular subject. it is a combined index of hearing ability and interpretation and the pure-tone audiogram plays no part in arriving at the index. while there is no practical value to assessment of percentage of hearing, the social adequacy index has a definite value and gives a truer reflection of "social hearing ability" than does a percentage figure b a s e d upon pure-tone audiometry. unfortunately, there are far too many factors which influence hearing and the s.a.i, takes into account only two of them, so that it cannot truly be regarded a s an index of social adequacy. the index can b e determined if speech discrimination does not reach the 100% level even at the highest amplification. it cannot be determined, however, if recruitment is present and the percentage discrimination actually decreases with an increase in amplification. malingering. numerous tests for feigning deafness have been devised and naturally some tests are better than others, but the malingerer simulating deafness may be difficult to detect with certainty. a new test is being carried out in sweden in which the speech audiometer is used and it a p p e a r s to b e almost impossible to simulate deafness effectively when this test is employed. the suspected malingerer is given an article to read and a tape recording of his speech is made. this recording is played back to the patient after a time lag of one third of a second. if he is in fact malingering, he finds it impossible to concentrate on what he is reading and begins to falter and stammer and becomes utterly confused within a few seconds. conclusion. i have attempted to show that speech audiometry should be regarded a s an essential part of modern otology. only a few years ago, otology consisted for the most part of surgery of suppurative ear disease. in the last 15 years, great advances in otologic surgery have coincided with advances in the treatment of infections of all kinds. contrary to frequently expressed opinion, the relief from treatment of suppurative ear conditions has made possible a greater concentration on the surgical treatment of deafness and has thereby widened the scope of otology. lempert designed the modern fenestration operation and thereby brought practical hearing within the reach of millions of sufferers from otosclerosis. hallpike described the pathology of meniere's disease and cawthorne designed his operation for labyrinthectomy which can be regarded a s a cure for unilateral meniere's disease. there is a considerable emphasis at the present time on the plastic surgery of the tympanic membrane and it is to be hoped that these procedures will b e productive of more useful hearing in the future. we have to await the results of the operation for mobilization of the stapes in otosclerosis and we may be on the brink of a further big advance here. because of the advances in otologic surgery, new standards have been set in all the auxiliary services and we have now reached the stage where speech audiometry is an essential auxiliary service. i do not suggest that b e c a u s e we have speech audiometry, that the older methods of investigation and examination may be discarded. on the contrary, i emphasise that complete examination of hearing now consists of clinical examination, including tuning fork tests, pure-tone and speech audiograms. it is only by accepting this a s a new standard in practical otology that we can hope to achieve the best results from management of the deaf patient, all the time. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) bevindings op die gebied spraakstem en gehoorgebreke by skoolgaande kinders deur i. c. uys, b.a.log. (rand;) b. prade, b.a.log. (rand). d e p a r t e m e n t s p r a a k h e e l k u n d e u n i v e r s i t e i t v a n p r e t o r i a . „ d i e t e r m d i a g n o s t i s e r i n g is v a n d i e w o o r d d i a g n o s e a f k o m s t i g , ' n m e d i e s e t e r m w a t b e t e k e n o m , d e u r d i e u i t k e n n i n g e n b e s t u d e r i n g v a n d i e s i m p t o m e , d i e a a r d v a n d i e s i e k t e t e b e p a a l . d i t is b e g r y p l i k d a t h i e r i n o o k d i e u i t k e n n i n g v a n d i e o o r s a k e v a n d i e s i e k t e o p g e s l u i t is m e t d i e o o g o p ' n d o e l t r e f f e n d e b e h a n d e l i n g d a a r v a n . m e t d i e o p k o m s v a n d i e p s i g o s o m a t i e s e g e n e e s k u n d e w o r d o o k d i e v o l g e n d e g e d a g t e in d i e b e g r i p d i a g n o s e v e r v a t , nl. d i e i n v l o e d v a n d i e s o m a t i e s e s t e u r i n g s o p d i e p s i g i e s e l e w e e n o m g e k e e r d . w e e n s d i e v e r s k e i d e n h e i d v a n d e n k s t r o m i n g s in d i e p s i g o l o g i e , is d i t b e g r y p l i k d a t d a a r g e e n e e n h e i d s b e e l d t e n o p s i g t e v a n d i e p s i g o l o g i e s e d i a g n o s t i s e r i n g o p g e b o u k a n w o r d n i e . " n a a a n l e i d i n g v a n b o g e n o e m d e is d i t d u s d u i d e l i k d a t o o k d i e s p r a a k t e r a p e u t m e t d r i e u i t e r s b e l a n g r i k e p r o b l e m e t e k a m p e h e t : 1. d i e p r o b l e e m v a n s u b j e k t i e w e i n t e r a k s i e t u s s e n p e r s o o n l i k h e d e . 2. d i e p r o b l e e m v a n s e m a n t i e k . 3. d i e p r o b l e e m v a n v e r s k i l l e n d e d e n k s t r o m i n g s in d i e s p r a a k h e e l k u n d e . v o o r d a t o o r g e g a a n w o r d n a o n d e r s o e k e g e d o e n t e r p l a a t s e , k a n d i e v o l g e n d e b e v i n d i n g s a s a g t e r g r o n d d i e n v i r ' n k o n t r o l e . bevindings in amerika 1. white house conference on childhealth and protection, special education, 1931. v r a e l y s j t e is r o n d g e s t u u r in s t e d e m e t ' n b e v o l k i n g v a n o o r d i e 10,000. d i e v o o r k o m s s y j f e r v a n s p r a a k g e b r e k e h e t g e v a r i e e r t u s s e n 1 9 . 0 2 1 . 4 % . d i e g e m i d d e l d e is d u s y a s g e s t e l a s 5 % . 2. opnames gedoen deur mills en streit i n t e n s i e w e o p n a m e s is in h i e r d i e g e v a l g e d o e n d e u r m i d d e l v a n i n d i v i d u e l e o n d e r s o e k e v a n e l k e k i n d . v a n a l l e s k o o l g a a n d e k i n d e r s is 10% m e t s p r a a k g e b r e k e g e v i n d . . e r n s t i g e s p r a a k g e b r e k e h e t v o o r g e k o m b y 4 5 % v a n h i e r d i e k i n d e r s . 3. opnames gedoen deur wilbert pronovost. p r o n o v o s t v i n d d a t 7 . 8 % v a n a l l e s k o o l g a a n d e k i n d e r s in n u e n g e l a n d ( v . s . a . ) s p r a a k g e b r e k e h e t . 4. bevindings van die a.s.h.a. komitee (1950 white house conference.) b e r e k e n i n g s w a t v o l g e n s d i e v e r s l a g d i e l a a g s t e is w a t w e t e n s k a p l i k v e r d e d i g k a n w o r d : 6 % v a n d i e t o t a l e b e v o l k i n g t o o n s p r a a k g e b r e k e . 5. opnames gedoen deur milisen. h i e r d i e v e r s l a g s p e s i f i s e e r m e r e n d e e l s d i e v o l g e n d e s p r a a k g e b r e k e : ( a ) d i s l a l i e : 1 5 % 2 0 % g e v i n d i n d i e g r a d e , m e t ' n d u i d e l i k e v e r m i n d e r i n g t o t d i e o u d e r d o m v a n 9 1 0 j a a r . ( b ) d i s f e m i e : ( s t u d i e s g e d o e n d e u r m i l l s e n s t r e i t 1942, b u r d i n 1940 e n r o o t ) . h u l le v i n d ' n n e i g i n g t o t t o e n a m e i n h a k k e l t u s s e n d i e 2 d e e n 5 d e g r a d e e n d a a r n a ' n v e r m i n d e r i n g in d i e p e r s e n t a s i e s . ( c ) d i s f o n i e : v a n d i e t o t a l e b e v o l k i n g is ( + — ) 1 % g e v i n d , t e r w y l 5 % 1 5 % v a n a l l e s p r a a k g e b r e k k i g e s o o k s t e m g e b r e k e g e t o o n h e t . bevindings in suid-afrika 1. opnames gedoen deur die universiteit van die witwatersrand. o p n a m e s w a t g e d u r e n d e d i e t y d p e r k 1962 1 9 6 3 in j o h a n n e s b u r g g e d o e n is, h e t d i e v o l g e n d e r e s u l t a t e g e l e w e r : (a) engelsmedium skole: g e m i d d e l d e a a n t a l k i n d e r s p e r s k o o l is 5 3 3 . g e m i d d e l d e a a n t a l k i n d e r s m e t s p r a a k g e b r e k e is 6 0 ( 1 1 . 8 % ) . (b) afrikaansmedium skole: g e m i d d e l d e a a n t a l k i n d e r s p e r s k o o l is 5 2 8 . g e m i d d e l d e a a n t a l k i n d e r s m e t s p r a a k g e b r e k e is 5 3 ( 9 . 5 % ) . v o l g e n s b o g e n o e m d e g e g e w e n s b l y k d i t d u s d a t d i e g e m i d d e l d e a a n t a l s k o o l k i n d e r s m e t s p r a a k g e b r e k e i n j o h a n n e s b u r g 10.7% is. 1 journal of the south african logopedic society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 2. opnames gedoen deur die universiteit o p n a m e ^ w a t gedurende die tydperk 1962 -1963 in pretoria gedoen is, het die voigende resultate gelewer: ( a ) per sfcool is 395 gemiddelde aantal kinders met spraakgebreke is 62 (18.2%). fh") afrikaansmedium skole: ( ) gemiddelde aantal kinders per skool is 504 gemiddelde aantal kinders met spraakgebreke is 75 (15.2%). die gemiddelde persentasie spraakgebrekkige kinders in pretoria blyk dus 16.7/0 te wees. b e s p r e k i n g v a n o p n a m e s g e d o e n i n s u i d a f r i k a 1. vraelyste is aan skoolhoofde gestuur waarvolgens onderwysers moontlike spraakgebreke kon uitken. 2 individuele onderhoude is gevoer in elke skool. die spraakterapeut het elke kind persoonlik gesien en 'n paar vrae gestel. na aanleiding van die resultate gevind met hierdie gekombineerde vraelys en individuele onderhoudsmetode, kon boge noemde afleidings gemaak word. voordele van hierdie metode. (a) 'n spraakterapeut sien elke kind persoonlik, dus is die moonhkheid dat η spraakgebrek ongesiens verbygaan skraal. (b) die onderwysers ken die kinders in hulle klasse beter, en aangesien hierdie persone die vraelys vooraf kry kan hulle die spraakterapeut bystaan in haar ondersoek. nadele van hierdie metode. (a) party kinders is skaam en praat nie ^ ; maklik met v r e e m d e mense me. dus bestaan die moontlikheid dat hulle spraakgebreke nie opgemerk sal word nie. (b) hakkelaars mag dalk oorgeslaan word ( omdat hulle soms vlotheidspenodes beleef. (c) breinbeseerde kinders is gewoonlik moeilik om dadelik te herken. (d) getalle mag groter wees as gevolg van onsekerheid. m o o n t l i k e r e d e s v i f i d i e h o e persentasie g e v i n d in pretoria 1 alleenlik laerskole, d.w.s. kinders van g aad l tot en met standerd 5 is ingesluit fn die opnames. soos genoem in vori^e studies, is daar 'n duidelike a f n a m i n spraakgebreke namate die kind ouer word. die vermoede bestaan dus dat die persentasie aansienlik laer sal wees as e s k o l e by die opnames sou ingesluit word. 9 dit is ook 'n bekende feit dat meeste van die kinders wat artikulasiegebreke toon gedurende die ouderdom van 5 jaar tot 8 f a a r dte probleem ontgroei selfs sonder spraakheelkundige behandeling. alle gradeklasse is ingesluit in die op names en dit kan waarskynlik vtr die hoe persentasie verantwoordelik wees. 3 soos vantevore genoem is, het die vraelys individuele onderhoudsmetode sy^nad y ele. aangesien die kinders net vir 'n tort rukkie gesien is, kan onsekerheid of selfs verbyga g ande fisiese of psigiese probleme l a s e i d i n g daartoe gegee het, dat die kind as 'n spraakgebrekkige bestempel word. die verskil tussen die tipe van opname gedoen ter plaatse, en die metodes wat in amerika gebruik is mag ook η invloed hg op die uiteenlopende resultate. 4. spraakheelkundige dienste is eebrei in pretoria nie en omdat omtrent feen opnames of behandeling vantevore h i l r toegepas is nie, is die moonthkheid s s spra g ak p gebrekklges reeds terapie ontvang het en gerehabiliteer is, in η mate uitgesluit. 5 i n 'n wetenskap soos spraakheelkunde het ons te kampe met die probleem van subjektiewe interaksie tussen persoonhkhede asook die probleem van semantiek. eile persoonlike ?iening en o f ntasie van elke spraakterapeut sal die resultate noodwendig moet beinvloed. 6 in die wye sin van die woord is p r e t 0 " £ 6 · meer eentalig as johannesburg. op die huidige stadium is dit nog me duideiik, indien wel, hoe hierdie feit die resultate van die opnames kan beinvloed nie. 7. alleenlik 'n minimum van skole kon gedoen word as gevolg van die beperkte stette die resultate wat dus we ver£ ν is kan nog nie as 100% geldig of vers n w o o r s i g e n d van die totale bevolking beskou word nie. 16 a u g u s t , 1 9 6 3 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) die behoefte aan verdere navorsing 1. dit is dringend dat 'n intensiewe studie gemaak word, waarin 'n beter verteenwoordigende aantal skole gebruik word. 2. vir 'n studie om geldig en waardevol te wees, is objektiwiteit belangrik en dit kan alleen verkry word indien 'n groter aantal spraakterapeute die opnames doen en sodoende die probleem van subjektiwiteit en persoonlike orientasie uit te skakel. 3. die behoefte word ook gevoel aan navorsing in verband met die waarde van die verskillende opnamemetodes en die verskil in resultate wat hulle meebring. 4. weereens mag dit interessant wees om navorsing te doen op die gebied van verskille tussen engelsen afrikaansmedium laeren hoerskole. 5. in die keuse van monsterskole moet die verskillende ekonomiese en sosiale standaarde van die gebied waarin die skole voorkom, in ag geneem word. ten slotte kan net genoem word dat hierdie studie in sigself alleenlik waardevol is as 'n aansporing tot verdere navorsing. opsomming 'n opname in verband met die persentasie van spraakgebrekkige skoolkinders is in pretoria gedoen en daarna vergelyk met die bevindings van opnames wat gedoen is in johannesburg en amerika. die persentasie is aansienlik hoer in pretoria. verskeie faktore kan as oorsaak genoem word, maar daar kan tot geen finale slotsam gekom word, voordat beter beheerde studies in verband met die saak gedoen is nie. die studie kan dus alleenlik dien as 'n aansporing tot verdere navorsing op hierdie gebied. summary a survey of the incidence of speech defects among school children in pretoria has been carried out and compared with the findings of surveys done in johannesburg and america. the percentage was found to be significantly higher in pretoria. a number of factors could be responsible for this fact, but no final conclusion can be reached until better controlled studies have been carried out. it is therefore felt that this study only serves the purpose of encouraging further research in this field. bibliography 1. van riper, c. speech correction, principles and methods. n.j.: prentice-hall inc., 1954. pp. 10, 33-36. 2. milisen, r. incidence of speech disorders. in ed. travis: handbook of speech pathology london: peter owen ltd., 1959. pp. 246-260. 3. — speech disorders and speech correction. white house conference report, 1950. j.s.h.d. 1952, pp. 129-137. 4. nel, b. f. sonnekus, m.c.h. opvoedkundige studies. no 33. negende jaargang. august, 1963 j o u r n a l o f the south a f r i c a n l o s o p e d i c society 17 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language development of the cleft palate child l o r e t t a h o r n , b.a. (sp. & h. t h e r a p y ) ( r a n d ) speech therapy department, general hospital, johannesburg summary the itpa was used in an assessment of the psycholinguistic abilities of three cleft palate children and three m a t c h e d normal children, of ages ranging from 48 m o n t h s to 66 m o n t h s . the data obtained revealed that the cleft palate children manifested a general depression in t h o s e areas sampled b y the itpa, in particular in areas testing expressive abilities. a transformational analysis of the language samples of the cleft palate children s h o w e d the relative immaturity of the syntactic structures used b y these children. in spite of the limitations of this s t u d y , there was a clear indication of a language retardation in the cleft palate group. several factors present in the early d e v e l o p m e n t of cleft palate children could adversely affect the acquisition of language skills. the implications of these findings for speech therapy are important. opsomming drie kinders met gesplete verhemeltes is vergelyk met drie normale kinders ten opsigte van psigolinguistiese vermoens deur middel van die itpa. die ouderd o m m e van die kinders het gewissel van 4 8 maande t o t 66 maande. die toetsresultate het a a n g e t o o n dat die kinders m e t gesplete verhemeltes oor die algemeen swakker gevaar het in die toetsgebiede van die itpa, in besonder in die items wat uitdrukkingsvermoe t o e t s . die analiese van die taalvoorbeelde van die kinders m e t gesplete verhemeltes was gebasseer o p 'n generatief-grammatiese benadering. hierdie analiese het g e t o o n dat die sintaktiese strukture soos gebruik deur hierdie kinders relatief o n o n t w i k k e l d was. nieteenstaande die tekortkominge van hierdie ondersoek is daar tog 'n duidelike aanduiding van vertraging in die taalstruktuur van die kinders met gesplete verhemeltes. die aanleer van linguistiese vaardigheid w o r d in die vroee ontwikkeling van hierdie kinders deur verskeie faktore b e i n v l o e d . die bevindings in hierdie verband is belangrik vir spraakterapie. literature on the subject of cleft palate and communication problems has been concerned primarily with the adequacy of articulation, and a great deal of information is available on the speech characteristics of individuals with clefts. most investigators have dealt with the speech output in terms of the defective elements which make it acoustically different from normal speech. discussions of cleft palate communication problems usually exclude information about language skills. only recently has attention been directed tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19 desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 loretta horn beyond the 'mechanics of the speech production of the cleft palate child to the fundamental language structures which are basic to communication. many problems associated with the occurrence of cleft palate, such as parental reactions to the defect, modified feeding methods, hearing loss, overprotection and lack of rewards for early speech attempts, and the unintelligibility of early speech, may have adverse effects on the development of language. clinical observation and parental reports have frequently indicated an apparent retardation in language acquisition, and some texts6· 2 1 make reference to the retardation of communication skills of cleft palate children. there has, however, been a paucity of systematic research into the language development of cleft palate individuals. relatively few studies have been carried out to experimentally investigate the language of children with clefts. it was felt, therefore, that consideration of this subject would yield useful information on the cleft palate problem and the factors influencing language acquisition. some investigators have attempted to study the language abilities of cleft palate children. bzoch3 reported a considerable delay in early speech developmental patterns in over 50% of his cleft palate subjects. further studies, directed more specifically towards language development, have yielded conflicting results. spriestersbach, darley and morris20 used measures of language such as mean length of response, structural complexity score and vocabulary size, and found that cleft palate children were retarded on measures of verbal output and vocabulary usage, but that they demonstrated no general language retardation. in a study using more extensive measures of language morris16 found that the cleft palate children were significantly retarded " . . .on every measure of communication skill for which comparisons with normal children were possible". smith and mcwilliams19 used the illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities to assess the language skills of 136 cleft palate children of three to eight years of age. they reported a depression in all nine areas of language evaluated. philips and harrison17 evaluated the language abilities of 137 cleft palate children and compared these with 165 noncleft children. they made use of the peabody picture vocabulary test, mecham's verbal language development scale, together with items selected from the kuhlman binet, the stanford-binet, the cattell, and the baker's detroit test of learning aptitude, as measures of language comprehension and expression. the children tested were between the ages of 18 and 72 months. the cleft palate subjects were found to be retarded in both receptive and expressive language abilities. the older cleft palate children (6 year level) were found to have a relatively greater degree of retardation in language skills, which implied that the language delay had not been overcome with maturation. they stressed the need for language stimulation programs for pre-school cleft palate children. from the studies mentioned above, it appears that children with cleft palates are often retarded to some degree in language development. decreased verbal output and more simple language structures have been reported. these findings support the hypothesis that the consequences of cleft lip and palate are much more than those of an anatomical defect alone. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language d e v e l o p m e n t of the cleft palate child 19 studies of this type have been based on quantitative measures of language which, although they did indicate gross developmental changes, did not yield information about the language structure as a whole. rather, they stressed different, isolated aspects of utterances without giving attention to the linguistic system which underlies the verbal activity of the speaker. it is this abstract system of rules enabling the individual to generate the sentences in his language, that linguists seek to describe. advances in linguistics, in particular the transformational model of grammar developed by chomsky5, have made possible more accurate and meaningful descriptions of language structure, and this has resulted in increased interest in the study of child language. the development of transformational generative grammar has had considerable influence on current research in language development. due to the limitations of the previously used language measures the present study included an analysis of language based on the transformational generative model, thereby taking into consideration the system of rules assigning structural description to utterances. in the present study the writer was concerned primarily with the children's acquisition of syntax, i.e. methods of combining words into sentences. in a surprisingly short period of time children acquire almost full knowledge of the grammatical system of their native language. it is postulated that in acquiring language the child incorporates a system of rules which formulates underlying regularities enabling him to generate utterances. the grammar of the linguist aims to describe the linguistic knowledge internalized by fluent speakers of a language; i.e. their linguistic "competence". the syntactic component of the grammar has different levels: phrase-structure level. at this level underlying strings which implicitly define the basic grammatical relations in the sentence are formulated by means of rules specifying the arrangement of lexical items. transformational level. more complex sentences are derived from base structure strings by the application of transformational rules, which involve operations of addition, deletion, substitution and re-arrangement of constituents. the distinction between deep structure and surface structure is central to the theory of transformational grammar. the deep structure of a sentence, which is the representation of the meaning of the sentence, is transformed by syntactic rules into a structure which is ultimately spoken or written, called the surface structure. morphological level. at this level inflectional rules, dependent on the previous application of sequential rules, are applied. in addition to the linguistic analysis of syntactic structures, a further measure of language skill was employed in the present study. in an attempt to compare the various psycholinguistic abilities of cleft palate and normal children, the illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities (itpa) was used. although this language test has been found to have limitations12 it appears to be a comprehensive test of children's language status. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 2 0 loretta horn factors influencing the language development of the cleft palate child. it is important to realize that the learning processes underlying the development of language are themselves dependent upon the existence and maintenance of certain conditions in the organism and environment. the psychosocial environment of the infant and young child during the years of language acquisition has to be taken into account when.considering the development of language. several psychosocial factors operating in the cleft palate population may have major impact on the total development of communication skills. from the very beginning the life experiences of the baby with a cleft are different to those of noncleft infants. appearance, feeding, dentition, surgery and general care often cause much concern. parental reactions of disappointment, guilt, defensiveness and resentment are frequently encountered. spriestersbach and sherman21 pointed out: .. .in few other clinical diagnoses is there as much likelihood of finding parental feelings of guilt, rejection and overcompensation, any of which would influence the interpersonal relationships between parent and child and in turn affect communication skills of the child. the reactions of parents to the birth of a cleft palate child have been investigated and reported.22 we must stress the importance of looking beyond the anatomical defect to the child himself and his parents. the role played by reinforcement in early language development has often been noted. the vocalizations of the cleft palate baby may not be rewarded to the same extent as those of the normal infant due to the lack of a warm, pleasurable relationship with the mother. artificial feeding methods, and the possible deprivation of normal oral experiences, together with a failure to satisfy the basic sucking and hunger drives of the infant can adversely affect the development of speech and language. a well recognized aspect of the cleft palate problem is the effect on speech intelligibility. it is felt that parental reactions to the early defective speech patterns may cause or aggravate the problems in language development. the utterances of the child with a cleft palate are often so different to those of the normal child, due to structural inadequacies, that they may be greeted with much less enthusiasm by parents. parents' failure to recognize early « utterances or even outright rejection of such vocalizations could result in deprivation of normal feedback and reinforcement. speech models and language stimulation through "imitation with expansion" by parents are felt to play an important role in language learning. brown and bellugi2 spriestersbach and sherman21 pointed out that in his failure to improve his approximations of the speech models he hears the cleft palate child may tend to withdraw from the activity and make fewer attempts to perfect his linguistic skills. parents may fail to expand the immature utterances of the cleft palate child because they do not expect better attempts from'the child, or because the unintelligibility of utterances makes such repetition and expansion impossible. 1 journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language d e v e l o p m e n t of the cleft palate child 21 it is common to find that cleft palate children have varying degrees of hearing loss. conductive losses often occur intermittently, especially at an early age. the possibility of a hearing loss during the important years of language acquisition has to be taken into account in the study of cleft palate children's language development. it is clear that there are several factors which could adversely influence the development of language in the child with a cleft palate. method the aim of the present study was to investigate the language skills of a group of cleft palate children. an experimental group of children with clefts, and a matched control group without clefts were compared with regard to their performance in the psycholinguistic areas tested by the itpa, and the complexity of syntactic structures used. subjects. the experimental group consisted of 3 children with complete clefts of the lip and palate (2 unilateral and one bilateral), of ages 48 months, 54 months and 66 months. these children were all being treated at the speech and hearing clinic, university of the witwatersrand. the control group consisted of 3 normal children aged 47 months, 51 months and 63 months. the children in the two groups were all boys, of matched socio-economic status. all had intelligence quotients in the normal to bright-normal range, and none had hearing losses of "educational significance"7 materials. the illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities was administered to each subject. certain modifications of the test were introduced as suggested by phillips18, who reported on the use of this test with english children. the stimulus materials used to obtain samples of language from the children consisted of two books with brightly-coloured pictures depicting everyday activities, toy motorcars, and a set of pictures chosen by the investigator to elicit certain responses. sentences consisting of vocabulary items present in the speech of young children, but containing syntactic structures more complex than those present in the sample of the child's spontaneous speech were, constructed for each child. for example, a child who gave no evidence of applying a passive transformation in the language sample obtained was presented with sentences containing this construction. the child was first required to repeat the sentence after the investigator, and thereafter he was asked to point to the appropriate picture, demonstrating his understanding of the sentence. such sentence imitation and comprehension tasks have been suggested as means of assessing the linguistic competence of children.9·1s: the distinction between competence and performance makes adequate assessment of tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 2 2 loretta horn children's linguistic levels difficult. the description which is of the greatest relevance is clearly the measure of competence, not that of performance. the majority of research studies have been based on linguistic descriptions of utterances produced, and to a lesser extent utterances understood by children at various stages of development. the present study is limited in that it is based, to a large extent, on performance. the repetition and comprehension tasks were applied to only some transformations, in an attempt to measure the competence of the child in these areas. procedure. the subjects were tested individually in familiar surroundings. the itpa was administered to each child, after hearing and intelligence had been tested. on another occasion the spontaneous speech of the child in response to the stimulus materials and the investigator's conversation was tape-recorded. the children were later presented with the repetition and comprehension tasks, which were based on the samples previously obtained. the speech sample of each child consisted of about 60 to 80 utterances. all utterances were taperecorded and later written out in traditional orthography. the language samples were then analyzed according to the transformational rules and restricted structures tabulated by m e n y u k 1 3 · 1 4 . the syntactic structures of the cleft palate children were compared with those of the normal children of matched ages. analysis of syntactic structures. the analysis of structures in this study was based on the model proposed by menyuk as a technique for describing children's grammar. the model is based on a generative grammar approach to linguistic structure. in the studies of m e n y u k 1 3 1 4 and brannon1 it was found that differences in levels of language development could be detected in terms of the transformational rules acquired, and the restricted structures present at successive age levels. there is similarity in the sequence of acquisition of structures through the developmental period. it was, therefore, felt that an analysis according to the rules exemplified in menyuk's descriptive model would yield information about the level of language development of the two groups tested. the samples were compared in terms of the use of the phrase structures, transformations and morphophonemic entities listed by κ m e n y u k 1 3 ' 1 4 . / menyuk's table of syntactic structures includes 14 simple transformations such as passive, negation, question and imperative, together with 12 generalized transformations such as conjunction, relative clause, complement and adjective. the list also includes various structures which are restricted to a children's grammar. on the phrase structure level these involve restrictions in the formation of the verb phrase, noun phrase, prepositions, articles and particles. transformational restrictions include contraction deletion., no question, pronoun restriction, etc., and morphological deviations in the verb form, noun form, possessive and pronoun are listed. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language d e v e l o p m e n t of the cleft palate child 2 3 results from the norms of the itpa the language age and standard scores for each subject were computed. the two youngest cleft palate children had language ages which were below their chronological ages. the language ages of the cleft palate group were all below those of the normal group. in order to compare the performance of the subjects on the various subtests the ranges were calculated for each subtest, as recommended in the itpa manual, mccarthy and kirk 1 l . two of the cleft palate subjects demonstrated a significant deficiency in the vocal encoding and motor encoding subtests, while the eldest cleft palate subject showed no significant weakness in these areas. the control subjects all showed relatively normal and stable patterns of performance in all areas, with no significant differences between the subtest performances. to enable comparison between the performances of the two groups a composite profile was plotted using the means of the standard scores for each different subtest. (figure 1.) the results of this study indicated that the cleft palate children performed significantly poorer than the normal children on the following subtests: vocal encoding, motor encoding, auditory-vocal automatic and visualmotor sequential. figure 1. indicates that the cleft palate subjects tended to show a general depression in the psycholinguistic areas tested by the itpa. analysis of syntactic structures. the description and classification of the linguistic data using menyuk's table of transformations and immature structures yielded valuable information about the syntactic structures present in the language samples of the two groups. clear differences were revealed between the language structures of the cleft palate children and the normal children. the most marked difference between the two groups was the large proportion of structures restricted to children's grammar appearing in the language samples of the cleft palate group. these were much less common in the samples of the control group. at the two youngest age levels tested such unique forms constituted over 50% of the utterances, whereas restricted forms accounted for a much smaller percentage of the normal children's utterances. at the phrase structure level there was frequent omission of noun phrases, articles and prepositions. the utterances were clearly indicative of rules applied in the early stages of language acquisition. the tendency toward the lack of expansion of a syntactic class on the phrase structure level, tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 2 4 loretta horn, representational l e v e l aut0m.-se0. decoding association encoding autcm beq . 1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 aud. v i s . audvoc v i s mot. voc mot aud-voc. audvoc. v i s mot . ,'v a / / s \ — · " — / ' . s / \ / \ / v \ \ \ % • / ν \ • \ \ / / • • \ « \ / φ + 3 . 0 0 + 2 . 5 0 +2.00 + 1 .50 + 1 . 0 0 + . 5 0 .00 . 5 0 1 . 0 0 1 . 5 0 2 . 0 0 2 . 5 0 3 . 0 0 normal group cleft palate group figure 1. profiles of the two groups (cleft palate and noncleft palate) on the itpa. resulting in an immature syntactic structure, was much more marked in the cleft palate group than in the normal group. there was clear evidence of late development of the auxiliary and model verbs, and the omission of the contracted form of the auxiliary "be" (contraction deletion) was often present in the samples of the experimental group. the language structures of the control group were more frequently well-formed, including correct expansion of the auxiliary node. at the morphological level there were omissions, substitutions and redundancies particularly of the verb form. in the use of generalized transformations the' normal children at the younger age levels were again superior. while the normal children frequently expanded sentences through the use of conjunctions, the cleft palate children showed journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language d e v e l o p m e n t of the cleft palate child 25 very limited ability in the use of conjunctions. restricted structures, involving omission of syntactic classes, violation of selectional restrictions and incorrect application of transformational operations were more numerous than correct transformational structures in the utterances produced spontaneously and in repetition by the younger cleft palate children. a marked difference, for example, between the control and experimental group was noted in the structure of interrogative utterances. wh-questions used by the cleft palate children were predominantly formed by the simple addition of an interrogative word to the declarative sentence structure, e.g. "where it is?" yes/no questions were formed by means of the application of intonational markers, without the inclusion of the auxiliary verb. these types of structures were clearly indicative of an early stage in the acquisition of transformational structures. the normal group revealed a greater linguistic competence through the earlier use and inversion of the auxiliary verb for generating correct question transformations. at the five-year age level the differences in the syntactic structures used by the two children were less marked. both had acquired basic syntactic structures, and there was no-significant difference between the base structures used by the normal child and the cleft palate child. on the transformational level the normal child used the correct form of the auxiliary verb more consistently, and gave evidence of greater syntactic competence in the application of generalized transformations. at each of the age levels considered in this study, but especially at the two younger age levels, the normal children gave evidence of more advanced linguistic competence through the decreased occurrence of restricted forms, and the greater proportion of grammatically correct transformations present in their language samples. discussion the cleft palate subjects in this study tended to show a general depression in those areas sampled by the itpa. the normal children were generally superior in the nine psycholinguistic areas investigated. figure 1. demonstrates the similarity in the shape of the profiles of the two groups, but clearly the difficulties of the normal subjects were not as marked as those of the children with clefts. the itpa revealed that the cleft palate children had a significant disability in the area of encoding, relative to their own performance in other psycholinguistic areas and in comparison with the performance of the normal children. the marked difficulty demonstrated in the vocal expression of ideas could be due to factors present in the early developmental period of the cleft palate children. less satisfying mother-child interaction, fewer speech models and decreased reinforcement of deviant speech patterns may have an adverse effect on the development of language skills. the possible effects of temporary hearing losses in the earlier stages of language development in these children tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 2 6 loretta horn must not be overlooked. the deficiency in the motor expression of ideas (i.e. gesture) can be associated with the general shyness, inhibition and dependency observed in the cleft palate group. these children are often overprotected by parents, and although parents might not view the child as "defective" they often feel he is "special" or "different" in some way, and therefore react to him in unrealistic ways. these children have often been found to be dependent on their parents, shy, and lacking in general spontaneity and initiative. another interesting finding was the significant difference between the performance of the two groups on the auditory-vocal automatic subtest. this is a form of grammar test requiring the use of increasingly less familiar english inflections, in a sentence completion task. to learn such grammatical rules the child requires adequate speech models, and it is postulated that a poor mother-child relationship, together with the unintelligibility of the child's early utterances could make the process of imitation with expansion less spontaneous, and the provision of speech models less frequent. these factors may also reduce the chances of reinforcement contingent on the child's vocal responses. these, and numerous other factors present in the early developmental period of the child with a cleft, together with the possibility of temporary hearing loss at some stage, could account for the deficiency found in this aspect of linguistic performance. the inferiority of the cleft palate children on the visual-motor sequential subtest has also been reported by smith and mcwilliams'19. they reported the consistent weakness of the cleft palate children in visual memory as an "interesting, although somewhat perplexing, finding". these writers considered the possibility of a general visual-motor deficit in cleft palate children. it appears that this area requires more extensive investigation. the results of this study are limited by the small number of children tested. it must be stressed that the results reported are not taken as being conclusive, but are merely an indication of the areas of difficulty found in the cleft palate population. developmental trends in the acquisition of syntactic structures have been described in studies by m e n y u k 1 3 · 1 4 and brannon1. the increased use of various transformational structures and decrease in the occurrence of deviant forms in the generation of structures during the normal process of language acquisition have been described. although a limited sample of language was obtained, and only selected syntactic structures were considered in this study, there was clear evidence of greater syntactic maturity in the language of the normal group as compared with the matched cleft palate group. the immature structures used by the two youngest cleft palate children were characteristic of the syntactic forms found in the utterances of much younger children. the occurrence of restricted structures, the late development of the auxiliary node, the lack of selectional restrictions and the limited use of transformational operations indicated that the cleft palate children were delayed in their acquisition of syntactic structures as compared with the control group. the writer feels that this immaturity of syntax could be journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language d e v e l o p m e n t of the cleft palate child 2 7 accounted for in terms of the unique problems associated with the occurrence of cleft palate. the possibility of undetermined degrees of hearing loss at earlier stages in the development of these children must be kept in mind. once again the failure of the cleft palate children to modify and transform the early patterns into more complex grammatical structures at the same age level as the normal children could be due to parents' failure to recognize or accept the early distorted speech attempts, resulting in deprivation of normal feedback and reinforcement. many of the factors present in the early developmental period of the cleft palate child could have an impeding effect on the acquisition of language skills. the results of this investigation give an indication of the problems involved in the development of communication skills in the cleft palate population. the findings reported here have significant implications for diagnostic and therapeutic procedures with cleft palate children. the assessment of children with clefts should include a thorough evaluation of language skills in addition to the traditional speech analysis. the therapist must be aware of the possibility of delay in language acquisition when counselling and interviewing parents of children with clefts. speech therapists are often concerned only with the adequacy of the speech production, and give no attention to the language abilities of the cleft palate child. in the consideration of treatment, the concept should be changed from speech correction to the facilitation of language development together with improvement of speech production. what the parents often need is information, together with counselling and emotional support. they need information about the cleft palate syndrome, as ignorance about such matters as etiology, feeding problems, surgery and speech therapy often leads to increased tension in the parent-child relationship. these parents also need information concerning language development. activities such as the provision of good speech models, continuous stimulation and motivation, the imitation of the child's utterances with expansion, and constant reinforcement of speech attempts come spontaneously within the normal parent-child relationship; in the interaction between the cleft palate child and his mother the spontaneous occurrence of these activities cannot always be assumed. the speech therapist must be prepared to train and advise mothers, and approach treatment with a knowledge and awareness of language acquisition processes. the habilitation program for the preschool child should include facilitation of language growth and development, and the positive aspects of inter-personal relationships through language should be emphasized. conclusion the descriptive method used in this study for investigating syntactic structures indicated a developmental immaturity in the language of the cleft palate group. the data obtained from the itpa results also indicated a general language depression in this group, with particular weaknesses in vocal tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 2 8 loretta horn expression, gestural output, auditory-vocal automatic abilities and visual memory in comparison with the normal children tested. it appears that there is some degree of language retardation in cleft palate individuals; but the factors responsible for this delay cannot be isolated. the generalization of these findings to pre-school cleft palate children on the whole can be made only tentatively, due to the limited number of children tested in this study. clearly, not all children with clefts exhibit a retardation in language skills. references 1. brannon, j.b. (1968): a comparison of the syntactic structures in the speech of 3and 4-year old children. language and speech, 11 (3), 171-181. 2. brown, r. and bellugi, u. (1964): three processes in the child's acquisition of syntax. in e.h. lenneberg (ed.), new directions in the study of language, m.i.t. press. 3. bzoch, k.r. (1959): a study of the speech of a group of preschool cleft palate children. cleft palate bulletin, 9(1), 2-3. 4. chomsky, n. (1964): formal discussion. monographs of the society for research in child development, 29(1), 35-39. 5. chomsky, n. (1965): aspects of the theory of syntax. cambridge, mass., m.i.t. press. 6. grabb, w.c. and rosenstein, s.w. and bzoch, k.r. (eds.). (1971): cleft lip and palate: surgical, dental and speech aspects. little, brown and company, boston. 7. hayes, c.s. (1965): audiological problems associated with cleft palate. proceedings of the conference: communicative problems in cleft palate. asha reports no.l, 83-90. 8. jacobs, r.a. and rosenbaum, p.s. (1967): grammar 2. ginn and company, boston. 9. lackner, j.r. (1968): a developmental study of language behaviour in retarded children. neuropsychologia, 6,301-320. 10. lee, l. (1966): developmental sentence types: a method for comparing normal and deviant syntactic development in children's language. /. speech and hearing dis., 31,311-330. 11. mccarthy, j.j. and kirk, s.a. (1961): illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities: examiner's manual. institute for research on exceptional children. univ. of illinois, urbana, illinois. 12. mccarthy, j.j. and olson, j.l. (1964): validity studies on the illinois test of psycholinguistic abilities. institute for research on exceptional children. univ. of illinois, urbana, illinois. 13. menyuk, p. (1963): syntactic structures in the language of children. j. child development, 34,407-422., 14. menyuk, p. (1964): syntactic rules used by children from pre-school through first grade. child development, 35, 533-546. 15. menyuk, p. (1969): sentences children use. m.i.t. press. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) language d e v e l o p m e n t of the cleft palate child 2 9 16. morris, h.l. (1962): communication skills of children with cleft lips and palates. j. speech and hearing dis., 5, 79-90. 17. phillips, b.j. and harrison, r.j. (1969): language skills of preschool cleft palate children. cleft palate journal, 6, 108-119. 18. phillips, c.j. (1968): the itpa: a report on its use with english children, and a comment on the psychological sequelae of low birthweight. british j. of communication dis., 3(2), 143-149. 19. smith, r.m. and mcwilliams, b.j. (1968): psycholinguistic abilities of children with clefts. cleft palate journal, 15, 238-248. 20. spriestersbach, d.c., darley, f.l. and morris, h.l. (1958): language skills in children with cleft palate. j. speech and hearing res., 1, 279-285. 21. spriestersbach, d.c. and sherman, d. (1968): cleft palate and communication. academic press, new york and london. 22. tisza, v.b. and gumpertz, e. (1962): the parents' reaction to the birth and early care of children with cleft palate. paediatrics, 30, 86-90. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e d i t o r i a l the field of speech therapy in south africa, as elsewhere, is dynamic and one must therefore expect changing ideas, needs and trends. in recent years there has been a growing awareness that we have based the practice of our profession on research studies that have been carried out elsewhere. the time has come for south african therapists to attempt their own research and become more acquainted with the unique conditions that exist in the country in which they work. s. bauman and m. aron have in their article indicated the need for research in south africa. we hope that this contribution will stimulate the initiation of many research projects. m. marks has brought to our attention another important need in our profession: namely the importance of a closer co-operation between allied professions in the treatment of speech disorders. her particular emphasis is on the need for a teamwork approach to cleft palate rehabilitation in south africa. south african therapists have always relied on the knowledge of those who have studied elsewhere. visitors from overseas and those of us who have travelled, have brought the knowledge that nutures our profession. a. g. epstein, having recently visited our country, presents in his article some new and interesting ideas on the testing of articulation disorders. in this issue we have therefore attempted to indicate some of the most urgent needs of speech therapy in south africa as well as presenting some new ideas. it is our earnest hope that further interest in these fields will be stimulated. i journal of the south african logopedic society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t o n a l responses o f m e n t a l l y r e t a r d e d b r a i n i n j u r e d c h i l d r e n b y betty hunt bradley columbus state school, columbus, ohio. in recent years there has been an increased interest in auditory disorders of children and this has been extended to include the problem of auditory disorders in mentally retarded braininjured children. the mentally retarded braininjured child may reveal some difficulties in responding to auditory stimuli or may exhibit communication problems, but there may be other children with similar etiological backgrounds responding to auditory stimuli or may exhibit communication problems, but there may be other children with similar etiological backgrounds who do not have these problems, but rather encounter difficulties in other areas such as visual perception. problem it is the purpose of this study to present auditory stimuli, tones played on an autoharp, and to determine if mentally retarded brain-injured children respond in terms of specific perceptual difficulties, or if. there is a similarity of performance related to other factors such as mental age. this investigation is related to attempts for experimental validation of certain descriptive classifications used in educational research classes with mentally retarded brain-injured children as a basis for differential teaching methods rather than employing a teaching curriculum based on the categorization of so-called typical behaviour of "the organic child." the theoretical background for this diagnostic teaching approach has been based on the formulations of dr. lise gellner who has stated that learning handicaps as well as mental retardation result from specific perceptual losses due to biochemical damage somewhere in the cerebral systems of vision and audition. a summary of her position stated in her booklet entitled, " a neurophysiological concept of mental retardation and its educational implications," is as follows: "mental retardation is due to different kinds of learning handicaps resulting from structural or biochemical damage somewhere in the cerebral systems of vision and audition. there is a dichotomy of the visual and auditory pathways in the brain which accounts for the existence of two central systems of vision and audition. one of these systems serves the integration of visual and auditory impulses emanating in the retina of the eye or the cochlea of the ear, with kinesthetic impulses from the somatic structures (muscles, joints, etc.) of the body; the other system serves the integration of siich visual or auditory impulses with impulses emanating from all autonomic structures (inner organs, blood vessels, etc.). each of these four cerebral systems extends, from the respective sense organs, via some important specific ganglia in the midbrain to specific projection area in the cerebral cortex. if these systems are intact, the result is normal seeing or hearing, but impairment anywhere in one or several of these four systems results in disturbance of function (gellner, 1959)." each of these disturbances is termed a disability since there is a direct negative effect on the performance of the child. it is gellner's contention that classroom materials and methods of presentationshould be based on the child's highest ability area rather than forcing responses to materials which are frustrating to him because of perceptual disability. this does not imply that gellner does not recognize that there are some auditory disorders caused by cortical lesions, but she feels these are not as severe in their effect on learning as brain stem lesions due to the fact that many times they do not affect both sides of the brain and adjustments and retraining can be introduced. methods for this study 70 mentally retarded children 6 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) who were residents of the columbus state school at columbus, ohio, were selected. the ss were divided.into two groups: (a) thirty five-mentally retarded children diagnosed as brain-injured and classified as having severe difficulties responding to auditory tasks such as language comprehension and verbal fluency, and (b) thirty-five mentally retarded children diagnosed as brain-injured and classified as having minimal difficulties responding to auditory and language materials. as far as can be determined, these children do not have sensory hearing loss. medical diagnoses indicated that brain damage was the primary cause of retardation. these diagnoses as to etiology were made by a classification committee of the receiving centre of the columbus state school. the 70 mentally retarded brain-injured children were selected from a larger group of children who had a diagnosis of brain-injury and who had also received classifications on the basis of criteria in her behavioral descriptions of the four disability groups. the perceptual abilities were determined by performance of the children on certain tasks or materials that gellner states are indicative of a disability area, for example, perseveration, echolalia, poor vocabulary, speech defect, and withdrawn behavior. the groups were matched with respect to ma, ca, and iq obtained on the stanford-binet intelligence scale. table 1 presents identifying information concerning the two groups. neither by matched groups nor by matched pairs are the group differences on ca, ma and iq statistically significant. the assumption of homogeneity of variance was met. electroencephalogram findings yielded additional evidence concerning factor of brain-injury within this group. the social histories indicated no other mentally retarded members of the immediate families. there was one exception to this whereupon other factors were felt to be responsible for injury to two members of one family. in many cases, although not all, there was a personal history of illness or injury to which the subject's condition was attributed. the basic procedure consisted of obtaining judgements on the difference of tones and tonal patterns played on the "new golden autoharp". the tones were presented three seconds apart and were of 2.6 seconds duration. the autoharp was selected because of its availability and the lack of familiarity with the instrument by the children. instructions for section a. "listen, i am going to play one sound on this harp." (examiner points to the instrument and demonstrates.) "now, i will make another sound. you tell me if these two sounds are the same, alike, partners, or if they are not the same, do not sound alike, are not partners. after the tones w e r e demonstrated, the e. said, "see, the sounds table 1. descriptive statisiics on ss. i i ca ma iq group { n range μ sd range μ sd range μ sd bi-ad i 35 9.1-21.3 15.30 2.8 5.7-11.3 7.75 1.5 38-79 55.91 9.4 h-nad 35 9.6-22.6 14.90 3.0 5.5-10.8 7.77 1.5 37-75 57.34 11.0 ad — auditory disability nad — no auditory disability r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) are the same." this was reversed for the opposite example and there were two examples administered for each section, one exemplifying the same tones or tonal patterns, one differences. instructions for section b. " i am going to play a little song on this harp. it has three sounds. listen.," e. demonstrates. "now, i am going to make a song with three more sound. you tell me if the songs are partners , alike, the same, or if they do not sound alike, are not the.same, are not partners." as in previous sections, two examples were given. instructions for section c. "listen, i am going to play some big sounds that go together. they are called chords." e. demonstrates. "now, i will play them again. you tell me if they sound the same, are alike, partners, or if they are not the same or partners." examples were given. if the ss failed to grasp directions, they were repeated one time. description of instrument. one test involving identification of tones and tonal patterns consisting of three sections was administered to each subject individually. section a was composed of 55 separate items each involving identification of two tones. twenty-five of these tones were identical or repeat tones and 30 tones were different involving tone differentiations of one tone as well as tones spanning two octaves. there were five items differentiated by one tone; nine items including tones within the same octave; ten items involving tones in concurrent octaves and six items involving tones separated by more than one octave. seventeen tones were directed upward in scale, i.e. from low to high, 13 downward, high to low. there were no tones given that were separated by exactly one octave. repeat items involved two bass tones, seven tones in lower octave, seven in middle octave, and nine tones in high octave range. section β consisted of identification of three tones. three tones were played, an interval, three more tones. five of these tones were repeated on second administration. all but one item involving differences included some tones of at least one octave separation. section c consisted of 10 chords, five involving repeat chords and five different chords. these were chords available on the autoharp and produced by pressing down on labeled key. two of the five chords were very similar involving fine discriminations, i.e. g major and g minor. results. results have been analyzed and presented in tabular form. table 2 shows the t ratio based on related samples for two paired groups of mentally retarded brain-injured children. the difference in scores between the mentally retarded brain-injured children with minimal auditory handicaps and the mentally retarded braininjured children with severe auditory handicaps table 2. performance of the matched pairs of mentally retarded brain-injured children on tone judgements. group mean sd t brain-injured nad ** 57.94 10.11 2.70 *** brain-injured ad * 52.03 8.3 * ad designates auditory disability. ; ** nad designates no auditory disability. *** ρ .05. the probabilties used are for a two tailed test of significance. 8 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) was significant beyond the .05 level. this allowed for a rejection of hypothesis i which stated in null form that: "mentally retarded brain-injured children without sensory hearing loss classified as having minimal difficulties in responding to auditory stimuli will not receive higher scores at a significant level (..05) than mentally retarded brain-injured children without sensory hearing loss having severe difficulties in responding to auditory stimulia bartlett's test revealed variances between the two groups to be homogeneous. correlations between performance of the groups and ca, ma and iq were insignificant, ranging from -.12 to .29. when a separate analysis was made in accordance with performance on each section, there were significant differences at the .05level on identification of two tones, section a; and identification of chords. section c, favoring the non-auditory group. differences were not significant for section b, three tones, but the trend was in the expected direction of higher scores for nonauditory mentally retarded brain-injured children. as was expected, the tone differentiations involving judgements of one tone separation were more difficult for both groups than tones involving differences of an octave or more. discussion. as gallagher has indicated in his book, the area of auditory perception has been almost neglected in terms of experimentation with braininjured and non brain-injured groups. werner and bower, in 1941, compared three groups of children: endogenous, exegenous, and normal, on their ability to reproduce melodic patterns. (ca: 6 years 10 years.) each child was asked to reproduce vocally a petternplayed on the piano. there were no differences noted in rate of errors but some subjective differences were noted in terms of organization of patterns, i there ha\|e been some studies regarding presentation of|auditory material to mentally retarded brain-injured children, but these seem more directly related to language development rather than judgements of tonal attributes. results of this experiment indicated differences in matched pairs of mentally retarded brain-injured children beyond the .05 level favoring the children with minimal, problems responding to auditory materials. there has been more discussion recently as to the need for individual tutoring of mentally retarded braininjured children based on their individual problems rather than relying on stereotyped behaviour patterns of these children. barnett, ellis and pryer suggest description of children iri terms of behaviour measures with the elimination of the term, "brain-injury." these results may indicate that the type of materials and the methods of presentation and stimulation may be a factor in their performance level rather than complete reliance upon etiological factors. however, in gallagher's sample the general mental ability factor accounted for approximately four times as much variance as did the perceptual factors. the results from this study in connectioc with other related studies seem to concur with gellner's suggestions that there is no one "organic" type of behaviour which is elicitec from mentally retarded brain-injured children. if hyperactivity, motor restlessness, mental age level, and clinical pictures, as described by strauss and bender, were primary factors in performance of mentally retarded brain-injurec children, one would predict similar scores from children matched on basis of etiological factors, ca, ma and iq. this did not occur. gellner thinks the problem of children who are described often as having difficulties with echolia, chattering, poor spontaneous speech, perseveration, word finding difficulty and difficulty in communicating especially with unfamiliar material and good speech patterns may have difficulty with musical tones due to injuries involving nuclei serving loudness and pitch. this assumption requires more experimentation and refinement with the present experiment, serving as an exploratory study involving identifications of tones rather than discrimination of pitch and loudness. summary. auditory stimuli, tones played on the autoharp were presented to two groups of mentally retarded brain-injured children. these children were paired on the basis of ca, ma and iq. they were differentiated into two groups: (a) thirty-five classified as having severe difficulties responding to auditory tasks such as language comprehension and verbal fluency, and q r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) (b) thirty-five children classified as having minimal difficulties responding to these materials. these groups were differentiated on the basis of criteria stated by dr. lise gellner as indicative of auditory disabilities. results showed differences significant beyond the.05 level suggesting higher performance level for mentally retarded brain-injured children with minimal auditory handicaps. there was little positive relationship shown between performance on tonal judgements and mental age as obtained on the stanford-binet intelligence scale and chronological age. the implications in terms of curriculum planning are discussed. opsomming. gehoorstimuli, tone gespeel op 'n outoharp is aan twee groepe van breinbeseerde, verstandelik-vertraagde kinders gebied. hierdie kinders is vergelykbaar volgens kronologiese, ouderdom, verstandsouderdom en i.k. hulle is in twee groepe verdeel. (a) vyf-en-dertig is geklassifiseer volgens ernstige probleme i.v.m. reaksie op ouditiewe take bv. taalbegrip en verbale vlotheid en (b) vyf-en-dertig is geklassifiseer omdat hulle minimale probleme op hierdie gebiede vertoon. hierdie groepe is gedifferensieer op gronde van kriteria wat deur dr. lise gellner gestel is as 'n aanduiding van ouditiewe orivermoens. die resultate dui op beduidende verskille vanaf die .05 peil wat weer op 'n hoer graad van optrede dui by die verstandelik-vertraagde kind met minimale gehoorsgebreke. daar is blykbaar geen positiewe verband tussen reaksie op toonbeoordelings, verstandsouderdom, soos deur die stanford binet intelligerisieskaal vasgestel en kronologiese ouderdom nie. die gevolg van hierdie bevindings ten opsigte van die beplanning van 'n kurrikulu m word bespreek. references. bamett, c., ellis, n.r., and pryer, margaret. "learning in familial and brain-injured defectives." amer. j. ment. defic. 1960, 64, 894. gallagher, j.j. " a comparison of brain-injured and non-brain-injured mentally retarded children on several psychological variables.." child dev. pub., 1957, monographs of the society for research in child development, inc., vol xxii, serial no. 65, 1957, no. 2, 17. gellner, lise. " a guide to the differential diagnoses of the four organic roots of mental deficiency," personal communication. gellner, lise. " a neurophysiological concept of mental retardation and its educational implications." chicago: levinson research foundation, 1959. hunt, betty. "differential responses of mentally deficient brain-injured children and mentally deficient familial children to meaningful auditory material." amer. j. ment. defic., 1960, 64, no. 5. patterson, ruth and hunt, betty. "performance of brain-injured and familial mentally deficient children on visual and auditory sequences. amer. j. ment. defic., 1958, 63, 72-80. werner, η., and bowers, m. "auditory motor organization in two clinical types of mentally deficient children." j. genet. psycgo., 1941, 59, 85-99. 10 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the investigation of the disintegration of phonemic discrimination on a perception and production level in adults with aphasia* j. levinsohn, b.a. log. (rand) it has been established for some considerable time that the role of audition in learning to speak is extremely important. the perception of auditory stimuli determines the child's ability to understand as well as to produce speech. cherry14 suggests that speech perception (i.e. understanding) and speech production, are the same phenomenon in the normal individual. schuell, jenkins and jimenez pabonir> go one step further and show how the auditory mechanism is also responsible for monitoring speech. the authors conceive of the language system as being dependent on the auditory system for processed information and regulation and control, mediated through feedback loops. the perception and production of speech sounds are based on the discrimination between the essential (phonemic) signals and the nonessential signals which are determined by the linguistic signals to which the child is exposed — this process is carried out by the auditory mechanism. (bauman1). luria1 2 explains that the auditory-articulatory system is responsible for learning of discrimination between speech sounds, which enables the individual to understand and produce speech. the link between perception and production in the normal person's language ability is stressed by luria: t h e p r o n u n c i a t i o n , i.e. t h e a r t i c u l a t o r y s t r u c t u r e of words, takes place on a basis of p h o n e m i c hearing: however, t h e articulation of sound itself plays an active role in t h e formation of p h o n e m i c hearing. modern investigators, basing their evidence mainly on clinical observation, show that it is this auditory mechanism that breaks down in the aphasic (ebbin2). schuell15 considers an auditory impairment to be basic to the aphasic's difficulty and concludes from a study on fifty-six aphasics that there is always some impairment of auditory processes in aphasia. she clarifies the nature of the auditory breakdown by explaining that the aphasic patient is unable to retain an auditory configuration or to summon it when required, i.e. there is a breakdown in the process of reauditorization. • b a s e d o n a s t u d y p r o j e c t p r e s e n t e d t o t h e s u b d e p a r t m e n t o f s p e e c h p a t h o l o g y a n d a u d i o l o g y , u n i v e r s i t y o f t h e w i t w a t e r s r a n d , 1968. journal of the south african logopedi society, vol. 16, no. , dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the investigation of the disintegration of 49 guttman,4 street,18 miller13 and karlin et al8 show, through experimentation, that some auditory impairment existed in the aphasic subjects tested. eisenson3 uses luria's explanation of the auditory analyser (situated in the secondary division of the auditory cortex of the left cerebral hemisphere) to explain that the process of auditory perception is not merely a passive receiving of stimuli but includes the analysis and integration of these stimuli. thus it has efferent as well as afferent functions. the auditory analyser is closely associated with the cortical apparatuses of the kinesthetic (articulatory) analysis. it has so far been established that: (a) audition is vital in the normal individual's understanding and production of speech; (b) many investigators feel an auditory disorder to be basic to the aphasic's language breakdown; (c) neurological findings confirm the link between perception and production both structurally and functionally. it seems feasible to postulate that dividing the aphasic's symptoms into those of understanding speech and those of producing speech is artificial, thus the receptive-expressive classification of aphasia is questioned. it seems possible that both receptive and expressive symptoms exhibited by the aphasic involve an auditory dysfunction. it has been felt for a number of years that some aspect of hearing accounted for receptive problems. wernicke's sensory aphasia was based on a loss of normal auditory control and kleist's word-sounddeafness occurred when the patient failed to appreciate speech sounds. it is only recently, that an imperfect auditory process has been used to explain the aphasic's expressive problems. keenan9 shows that where the auditory stimuli of language cannot be retained, both understanding and expression of language become impaired. thus receptive and expressive language impairments are not different forms of aphasia, but rather different manifestations of one underlying impairment. if audition is basic to the aphasic's problem of expression, and if the auditory perception centre and the articulatory centre are closely related, the articulatory errors made by an aphasic subject should be similar to his errors in the perception of phonemes, i.e. the phonemic disintegration shown by an aphasic subject should be the same on a perceptual (intake) and production (output) level. on this hypothesis the following experiment was carried out. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 50 j. levinsohn procedure subjects. seven aphasic subjects were used in the experiment. they formed a fairly representative sample as the major variables of sex. age, educational level and economic standing were represented. as one test required standardization on a normal population, six 'normal' subjects were also used and were chosen so as to represent the variables mentioned above. aphasic subjects. s a β c d ε f g sex .. f μ μ μ μ μ f age . 28 52 38 30 60 67 20 education .. st. 8 univ univ univ m a t r m a t r matr economic level .. l u u μ l m μ u m normal subjects. s a β c d ε f sex f μ f f μ μ age 21 19 45 60 44 58 education .. univ univ st. 8 m a t r matr u n i v economic level .. u μ lm u l μ abbreviations: univ: university education u: upper income group m: middle income group l: lower income group u m : upper-middle income group l m : lower-middle income group f: female m : male. tests used. a battery of tests was given to each aphasic,subject: (a) hildred schuell's short examination for aphasia.1' this was used to determine whether the subject was aphasic and, if so, what type. the following aspects of language were examined: (i) auditory: this includes tests of i auditory recognition, retention span and comprehension. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the investigation of the disintegration of 51 (ii) reading: tests of visual and auditory word recognition and reading comprehension were included. (iii) speech and language per se: here tests of cranial nerve involvement, sensorimotor involvement and functional speech were included. (iv) writing: the subject was tested on revisualization, spelling and functional writing. the scores obtained by the subjects determined to which of the five groups they belonged. schuell states: probably 90% of aphasic patients fall into one of the five main groups, when classified according to pattern of impairment. schuell offers the following explanation for each of the five groups: group (i): this is characterized by an almost total loss of all language functions. group (ii): only auditory processes are impaired, but the impairment is reflected in defective speech, reading and writing. group (iii): shows the same pattern as group (ii) but is complicated by specific visual impairments. group (iv): there is an involvement of auditory and sensorimotor processes. group (v): scattered auditory, visual and motor (usually cranial nerve) impairment compatible with generalized brain damage is found. some language is usually retained. (b) pure-tone audiometry. according to the specific hypothesis of this study, tests were needed which would determine the subject's hearing acuity, thus enabling the experimenter to establish whether phonemic errors, if present, were caused by a hearing loss or a perceptual problem. thus an audiometer was used to establish the subject's threshold of detection for pure-tones. this was also necessary as a control measure. knowledge of the subject's pure-tone threshold would enable the experimenter to compare this with the speech reception threshold and the speech discrimination results, and establish whether there was any similarity. (c) audiometric speech reception thresholds: this test establishes the threshold at which a subject is just able to hear speech. this is essential as it must be known whether the words in a phonemic discrimination test were inaccurately perceived or merely not heard. (d) phonemic discrimination perception test. the hutton, curry and armstrong semi-diagnostic test" was used. this particular test was chosen as it appears to be well standardized, includes discrimination between vowels as well as consonants, and the words used are tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 52 j. levinsohn fairly common ones. the purpose of a perception speech discrimination test in this study is to establish the aphasic's ability to distinguish between the phonemes presented aurally. the subject was required to mark off on a sheet of words grouped in fours, the stimulus word e.g. bowl, bail, ball, bull formed one group, and bowl was the stimulus word. (e) phonemic discrimination production test. the schuell picture test for consonants and the picture test for vowels were used.17 both tests consist of a series of pictures. each card contains two pictures and the names of the pictures differ from each other by one phoneme only; i.e. the names are minimal pairs, e.g. pea and bee. these form one series of the consonant test and shed and shared form one series of the vowel test. from these tests, the aphasic's ability to distinguish between phonemes aurally was assessed. (f) it was considered interesting to note whether the hypothesis could be extended to include discriminations produced in spontaneous speech. thus, a sample of each subject's speech was recorded and errors transcribed. an attempt was made to keep the testing procedure standard for all subjects. the physical environment, instructions and rest periods were similar for each subject. each subject was tested over two sessions to avoid fatigue, and the order of test presentation was constant. a two-room set-up was used for the phonemic-perception discrimination test. results (a) the results of normals on the vowel discrimination test: all subjects were able to discriminate adequately between the phonemes presented by the pictures. (b) results of the shorter schuell test: all seven subjects showed aphasic symptoms, and could be classified into one of the five schuell groups. as can be seen all groups, except group one, were represented. subject schuell group a 2 β 4 c 5 d 3 ε 3 f 2 g 2 summary of symptoms for / each group / only auditory processes impaired involvement of ' a u d i t o r y and sensorimotor processes scattered auditory, visual and motor impairment auditory processes impaired plus specific visual involvement as d above only auditory processes impaired as f above journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e nvestigation of the disintegration of 53 (c) results of hearing tests: summary table of each s u b j e c t ' s audiogram, average level of speech range and speech reception threshold. a verage intensity speech receptii on s age pure-tone hearing for speech range threshold a 28 right ear: normal right ear: 8 db right ear: 5 db lift ear: normal left ear: 8 db left ear: 0 db β 52 right ear: 8000 cps right ear: 0 db right ear: 0 db at 25 db right ear: left ear: normal left ear 2 db left ear: 0 db c 38 right ear: 8000 cps right ear: 3 db right ear: 0 db at 50 db right ear: left ear: 8000 cps left ear: 13 db left ear: 10 db at 40 db d 20 right ear: normal right ear: 7 db right ear: 0 db ί eft ear: normal left ear: 3 db left e a r : 0 db ε 60 right ear: 4000 cps right ear: 7 db right ear: 0 db at 40 db 8000 cps at 50 db l.eft e a r 4000 cps left ear: 8 db left ear: 10 db at 45 db 8000 cps at 70 db f 67 right ear: 4000 cps right ear: 13 db right ear: 35 db at 50 db 8000 cps at 55 db left e a r 4000 cps left ear: 32 db left ear: 40 db at 55 db 8000 cps at 65 db g 21 right ear: normal right ear: 1 db right ear: 10 db left ear: 4000 cps left ear: 26 db left ear: 25 db at 25 db 8000 cps at 50 db five out of seven subjects showed normal bilateral hearing. the remaining two showed a unilateral loss in the left ear with a conductive-type loss for the speech frequencies. one could, therefore, expect almost a 100% speech discrimination score for all the subjects. four out of seven subjects showed a sensori-neural type of loss in the high frequency range. it seems unlikely that this is caused by presbycusis as the mean age of the subjects showing this was forty-six years. also, as the case histories of the subjects do not reveal any possibility of a noise-induced loss, it was hypothesized that the loss was the result of the aphasic condition rather than any specific auditory disorder. the subjects showed a lower hearing threshold for pure-tones than for speech. this points to cortical pathology (which is known in these subjects) and also the possibility of speech discrimination difficulties. (d) similarity between phonemic discrimination on a perception and a production level was shown. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 54 j. levinsohn "α ρ fu 0 κ tq c a "a ~ ε 5 a ο a, e ο 3 -q ο a ft. c. υ <1 . ο a η. ο <1 £ 3 ό ο ό c λ > .η ο, .2 tλ rt -ο ίλ 3 c ϊ ο j • g>° "λ ε s λ ο ii-jjjbo λ u ϊλ & 3 c ή ° ί h e * ο c ο λ ο ιj i f f l μ ο λ _ο c . ο •ο ".β (λ 3 c •2*0 μ c "j u c u rt ο >j j s e a c λ c ο (λ c ο ο 3 •ο ο •ο c π) c. ο ο § | · c c ω> c ο c ί ο •ο μ ct ο ι* m c υ .β (λ •3 "ο ο c. — '3 j) c tn c c u c ο •ο i α 1 ω ' . . „ ρ3 cq ι ξ c s ο ο u ό _ ο c • ω ο > λ 5 c ω "2 ce •ο τ3 ο c " cs "rt ή c c hh π ; 3 •ό c 5 ο ™ λ ο c > 3 •ο ο ό c ; ω ο ι s o 3 — α α c c cd ^ -ο 5 ω . ο α> (α ί> > u> 'δ r« ο. c (λ · cj (λ 71 3 c «2 | ι ι ε c « c ο s <υ ι r-l m νη \£> γνu 3 ο · ο (λ γ3 ο (λ « ο ϊ ο ω « •β» ^ g a a s * σζ ο rj c m •s ^ μ ° 8 "2 •o -9 « ω υ g-" •ξ .ξ ο c ί ο •ο μ c λ ο u •ο a έ ce 5 3 ® s ϊ ιg ιμ ca i = c a s ω c c u rt ο ο γ 1 c ο . '2 υ u ·3 ρ (λ ίλ cd .β _ο ^ c c. c ' 2 · c 5 ο α ο ο c ο ^ u οι ό u s u hj chj tvrf ^ ili o h j: —1 ce cq m η c ct c ο (λ c ο ο journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e investigation of the disintegration of 55 a 0 κ 3 3 >"a kj c a ε c a f 's. "a ο ft, c g q; a n. ο «2 ο k. k. 3 ό ο c ο ο c c ω ί ί , ο οτ -α -ο rt cz -i. ω u s tο m m > —' rj c bo ο c i g c c ω ί ί , ο ο + τ3 -ο ω ω ? ' ο > mm e 6 0 ρ s ίλ js £ "ω ιc §ξ c c cs ω ο οτ ό ό d ra > ο ω ρ mm > ο > s υ ο j ο, c 2 .2 u ^ 2 c ε •3 λ ε<« ή 2 3 ιυ (λ •a u 2 η s ε tο ω · γι ο u >dh ζ π) •ο π) ε ο ζ 8 s ..ϋ g •—ο ce "· < e c «η ω ε < ° ί · 2 ο-^.ξ s . g | α» ο u l> " ω c ; s o § * £ ^ ω r. μ" « • α » , » c ξ*° § s ο e g c ο * a a 2 t c ra rt aj i j j f t ο ^ c*i r^ ζ u c nj c ο .a is c ί ο •ο " ίλ c >, cs ~ s ο ° ο 2 •ο c λ c ο ίο ce g ή •c ο •3 3 ω ο β; c λ ο ο c ο ίλ c ο . 3 « e '3 2 | 8 m s ^ s . 3 5 2 s g θ ό · ε ά , ε . s h s — r-t -ο c 2 3 > am π g i ° «1 •6 c ο μ ° ο ο y c i"3 λ ο m 2 i . e χι ct ο χ .& ε · a · ε ο · _ ιλ d •s ή α _ w c 3 ο —' 5ρ •q ιλ ο ο u i j : « ri c ct c ο (λ c ο ο •ο ' 2 c g ι. ο ί 3 · ο η οτ3 ο 3j< c12 £ υ β,βο " rj •g ο ω c ο m ο -κ ω> γ— ιμ λ α) c . s 5 • " ε ο c 'c τ-, * 8 c ο^[β -ο ^ ι"u 2 2 * ιο ο m ο-ι > ο > tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 j. levinsohn "s i z* s5 ^ "o tl -21 "a kj c a .2 hj ft. -a ο •3 ho s a ft. « κ a ft. a to q, (λ w ω ο >-λ i ο ό μ α <υ ucq ο ό μ cq ω 3 ό ο > c a ο c •ο .2 ω w !υ ο α, λ -α cq . ο <υ cd ά. ο « ο ο cl, η •ό > v— <4~ α ε ο. ο £ u μ c . c τ3 1ο u of | ^ ιλ u τ3 χ> λ + 3 ,2 υ υ « £ £ 5 journal of the south african logopedi society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the investigation of the disintegration of . 57 ζ ο ρ υ 3 ο g α. ο ζ υ cc ω cc υ u) υ σ ω ζ ο ι a ? ί-α) α. < -ι | 55 σ σ 3 ιλ .6 ζ ~ "3 -α ^ δ <υ s « -ο o h 2 β co α a ttj c 3 . 0 1*1 o. "a ο •ο ο ^ 3 ft. •ο ho ΐ «2 u1 ο to υ kh kh • o > ί -α ο .ο is c ϊ ο ό μ cq ο υ _> l· c ? ο « ό % co w ω c ο ο . ί η .5 μ j λ — (ν χ cd ^ cd rt ο 4 > c s c 1— flj cq-s — (ν s λ .5 ϋ '•3 ή ^ s » § ό. o -s ω ο 0) > ο u •ό _ •χ cd cd ~ υ c ιu cq ό λ χ ο υ _> cd ά c ? ο — ό % μ 2 cd s c £ ο cq υ χ cd ε . · — (λ 3 "ΐί ο ο ω ο u ε 3 . s c ? ο ό 2 μ 2 cd w u c λ ο cq ο cd ι1 1 ο υ ό _ •a a ω c cd χ c ? ο ό cqt3 cq ο · (λ cd ή α. ο | _ο c υ ε « ο —1 ε . (λ ii. u · (λ cd ^ α ο ω _ο υ ε cd ο •sjs fc-5. r * 1λ tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol.16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 58 j. levinsohn discussion (a) intra-subject responses on perception and production levels tended to be related. this was seen either as the same substitution of one phoneme for another on the perception and production tests or as a trend or error pattern observed in both tests (e.g. a breakdown in front:back contrast of consonants). the degree of similarity differed for each subject and although all showed some phonemic breakdown which existed on both the input and the output levels, all subjects but one also showed phonemic breakdown on one level and not on the other. however, owing to the limitations of the tests used (to be discussed more fully later) although the results do not support the hypothesis fully, neither do they negate it. (b) little similarity existed between phonemic errors in isolation and in spontaneous speech. (c) a number of relevant factors, not specifically related to the hypothesis, were noted: (i) the subjects' phonemic errors were not random and inconsistent but followed a phonemic trend or pattern. this is in accordance with schuell's results.1"' (ii) there was a strong similarity between auditory-type aphasic symptoms and phonemic discriminatory ability. (iii) the severity of the aphasia seemed closely linked to the degree of phonemic breakdown. diagnostic and therapeutic implications (a) a full audiometric assessment should be one of the tests used for testing aphasics. i (b) if some form of auditory breakdown is evident in all aphasic patients it is likely that other cases of cerebral dysfunctioning also have neural auditory impairments. : (c) if auditory perception and spieech production are linked, auditory training with patients should be done. accurate listening and phonemic discrimination should aid speech production and influence the use of language. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the investigation of the disintegration of 59 limitations and evaluation of study (a) only seven aphasic subjects were used in the experiment. it is impossible to draw conclusions and generalize test findings based on so small a sample. (b) the scope of the study did not allow the author to draw up identical tests of phonemic discrimination for perception and production. factors such as the phonetic environment of the phoneme being tested and the fact that blends were included in the perception test but not in the production test, reduce the reliability of the tests used. (c) as aphasics are known to show perseveration difficulties it is possible that a repeated response was noted as a phonemic error. (d) as the phonemic discrimination tests demanded a word-naming ability, it was difficult to discriminate, with some subjects, whether the difficulty was one of word-naming or of phonemic breakdown. (e) one cannot state definitely that the disintegration of phonemic discrimination was the result of a cortical or a retrocochlear disorder. (f) the fact that little similarity was shown between perception and production test results and spontaneous speech can be attributed to the fact that the samples of speech elicited were inadequate as they were too short. implications for research (a) the results of this study as well as experimental evidence and clinical observation seem to indicate that auditory impairment may be the core of most aphasics' difficulties. however, the exact nature of how audition affects or causes other aphasic symptoms is not understood. in terms of the particular symptom analyzed in this study, further experimentation is needed to determine the relationship between auditory perception and a breakdown in phonemic discrimination on an articulatory level. (b) it is extremely important to establish at what level of auditory perception the aphasic breakdown occurs. hirsch,5 offers four levels of perception: (i) detection. this is the threshold at which sound is just heard. (ii) discrimination. the listener compares one speech sound with another. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 60 j. levinsohn (iii) recognition or identification. the listener compares incoming sound stimuli with his memory of sounds and words. (iv) comprehension, i.e. recognition sustained over a long period of time. the testing of an aphasic patient on these four levels would also test the functioning of the auditory analyser (a term used by luria1 2) to note whether the auditory defect was one of analyzing, comparing, or synthesizing received auditory stimuli. (c) all subjects in this study showed some degree of phonemic breakdown. does this breakdown mirror the child's acquisition of phonemic discrimination? jakobsen ar.d halle7 state: the linguistic, especially the phonemic, progress of the child and the regression of the aphasic obey the same laws by implication. if this were indeed so, one could determine the aphasic's level of phonemic disintegration and assign this to the particular developmental level of phonemic competence of the child. therapy would aim at teaching the aphasic the next and following stages through which the child normally progresses. summary as more stress is being placed on the auditory disability of aphasics, the validity of dividing aphasic symptoms into expressive and receptive disorders is queried. the reflex-arc seems too simple a configuration to explain the complex functioning and breakdown of language. it is postulated that the auditory disorder is basic to aphasic symptoms on the level of understanding as well as that of production of language. thus, if an aphasic patient shows a breakdown in the perception of phonemes, it seems likely that the auditory imperception will affect the production of the same phonemes. aphasic subjects were presented with tests of phonemic discrimination on a perceptual and a production level and the similarity of phonemic errors was noted. the results of the experiment seemed to indicate that^a' hearing loss did not account for the subject's phonemic disintegration. however, it was not possible to control certain factors thus this result is not conclusive. the similarity between errors on an input level and those on an output level was poor, according to the result of the perception and production tests used in the study. however, despite the inadequacies of the tests used, all subjects showed'some degree of similarity and this tends to support the hypothesis. the errors in phonemic journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 16, no. , dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) the investigation of the disintegration of 61 discrimination indicated by both perception and production tests were not random and inconsistent, but followed a trend. it was noted that the severity of the aphasic symptoms seemed closely linked with the degree of phonemic breakdown. also, the subjects tested showed a significant similarity between auditory-type aphasic symptoms and phonemic discriminatory symptoms. little similarity existed between phonemic errors in isolation and in spontaneous speech. opsomming aangesien meer klem geplaas word op die ouditiewe onvermoe van afasie gevalle, word die geldigheid van 'n verdeling van afatiese simptome in ekspressiewe en reseptiewe versteurings in twyfel getrek. die refleksboog blyk 'n te eenvoudige konfigurasie te wees om die komplekse funksionering en afbraak van taal te verklaar. dit word veronderstel dat die ouditiewe versteuring onderliggend is aan afasie simptome op die vlak van begrip asook op die vlak van taalproduksie. as 'n afatiese pasient dus 'n afbraak toon in die persepsie van foneme, blyk dit dan waarskynlik te wees dat die ouditiewe onvermoe die produksie van dieselfde foneme sal aantas. fonetiese diskriminasietoetse is op 'n perseptuele sowel as produksie vlak gegee, en die ooreenkoms van fonemiese foute is aangeteken. die resultate dui daarop dat 'n gehoor verlies nie die oorsaak is van die geval se fonemiese disintegrasie nie. sekere faktore kon nie konstant gehou word nie, dus is daar nie afdoende bewys vir hierdie resultaat nie. volgens die uitslag van die persepsie en produksietoetse wat gebruik is in die studie kon afgelei word dat die ooreenkoms tussen foute op ontvangsvlak, en die op vlak van weergawe swak was. ten spyte van die ontoereikendheid van die toetse wat gebruik is, het alle proefpersone 'n mate van ooreenkoms getoon en dit ondersteun die hipotese. die foute in fonemiese diskriminasie wat aangetoon is deur beide persepsieen produksietoetse was nie lukraak en onkonstant nie, maar het 'n definitiewe lyn gevolg. ' daar is opgemerk dat die erns van die afatiese simptome nou verwant blyk te wees aan die graad van fonemiese afbraak. die proefpersone wat getoets is het ook 'n beduidende ooreenkoms getoon tussen ouditiewe-tipe afasie simptome en fonemiese diskriminatoriese simptome. klein ooreenkomste het bestaan tussen fonemiese foute in isolasie en spontane spraak. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 16, nr. 1, des. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 62 j. levinsohn references 1. bauman, s. (1964): an organismic approach to some concepts of dysphasia with particular reference to the work of a. r. luria and soviet scientists. paper presented at the conference on aphasia at the university of pretoria. june 1 6 t h 1 7 t h , 1964. 2. ebbin, s. c. b. (1967): speech sound discrimination of aphasics when inter-sound variable is varied. journal of speech and hearing disorders. 10, 1. 120. 3. eisenson, j. (1968): developmental aphasia: a speculative view with therapeutic implications. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 33, 3. 4. g u t t m a n , b. j. (1967): investigation of speech reception and discrimination in adults with aphasia. unpublished study project for b.a. log., university of the witwatersrand. 5. hirsch, i. j. (1966): auditory perception of speech. vol. iii. ulca f o r u m in medical sciences, 1966. 6. hutton, c., curry, ε. t. and armstrong, μ. b. (1959): se'mi-diagnostic test materials for aural rehabilitation. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 24, 4, 321. 7. jakobsen, r., and halle, m. (1956): fundamentals of language. m o u t o n & co., gravenhage. 8. karlin, i. w., eisenson, j., hirschenfang, s., and miller, m. (1959): a multi-evaluational study of aphasic and non-aphasic right hemiplegic patients. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 24, 369. 9. keenan, j. s. (1968): the nature of receptive and expressive impairments in aphasia. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 33, 1. 10. licklider, j. c. r. (1952): on the process of speech perception. journal of the acoustic society of america, 24, 6. 11. liebermann, c. s., cooper, r., shankweiler, b. and studdert-kennedy, b. j. (1967):' perception of the speech code. psychological review, 74, 6. 12. luria, a. r. (1958): brain disorders and language analysis. language and speech, 1, 14. 13. miller, μ. h . (1960): audiological evaluation of aphasic subjects. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 25, 333. 14. price, l., david, c. s. and goldstein, r. (1965): abnormal bikesy tracings in normal ears. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 139-144. 15. schuell, h., jenkins, j. j. and jimenez-pabon, e. (1965): aphasia in adults. hoeber medical division, h a r p e r and rowe, n.y. and london. 16. schuell h . (1953): audiological impairments in aphasia. significance and retraining techniques. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 18, 14. 17. schuell, h. (1967): a short examination of aphasia. neurology 7(a), 1957. 18. street, b. s. (1967): hearing loss in aphasia. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 22, 60. 19. weisenberg, t. and mcbride, m. (1965): aphasia — a clinical and psycho logical study. oxford university press, n.y. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 16, no. 1, dec. 1969 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sajcd 218 editorial this edition of the sajcd showcases some of the best research currently being carried out in the field of communication sciences and disorders, both locally and abroad. you will find here a stimulating mix of audiology and speech pathology papers – linked together by the developing contexts in which the work was carried out, and the desire to build the evidence base for our work in this context. i am extremely proud of the journal’s growth over the last few years – both in terms of the number of papers submitted (we are very often run off our feet as we try to manage them!) and also the quality of the papers and the peer reviews we receive. our website http://www.sajcd.org.za/index.php/sajcd/index has almost 300 registered users who regularly download papers, carry out reviews or submit papers of their own. in addition to these regular users, we have an impressive set of stats about the number of ‘casual’ visitors to the site each day and the frequency with which papers are downloaded by interested parties from all over the world. our small journal is widely read. the editorial board has a responsibility to support and develop the academic writing and reviewing skills of all interested members of the profession. this is a commitment that we take seriously, especially given the small size of our professions and our developing context where funding for postgraduate training and research support is often limited. in some cases, an editor (together with input from anonymous peer reviewers) may work intensively with first-time authors – sometimes over rather extended periods of time – to ensure that a paper is ready for publication and meets the required standard of the journal. we are proud of the support we have given and the generous feedback we receive about this developmental approach – it makes the editorial process worthwhile. this will be my last volume of the sajcd as editor. it is time for another editorial team to take over. i want to thank my co-editors: vivienne norman who has given of her time and expertise so generously and efficiently over several years, and dunay taljaard who acted as a co-editor for this year’s edition. our editorial board consists of a stalwart group of individuals who support and advise the core editing group; thank you for your support, wisdom and dedication. peer review is a key component of high-quality scholarly publishing; reviewers are asked to make a considerable investment of their time and energy in carrying out this work. thank you to everyone who has carried out peer reviews for the journal over the past few years – your contributions are greatly appreciated. michelle pascoe editor-in-chief abstract background to the research survey the research method results of the survey discussion and implications of the results conclusion acknowledgements references footnote about the author(s) thandeka mdlalo speech therapist, livingstone school, durban, south africa penelope flack discipline of speech language pathology, university of kwazulu-natal, south africa robin joubert discipline of occupational therapy, university of kwazulu-natal, south africa citation mdlalo, t., flack, p., & joubert, r. (2016). are south african speech-language therapists adequately equipped to assess english additional language (eal) speakers who are from an indigenous linguistic and cultural background? a profile and exploration of the current situation. south african journal of communication disorders, 63(1), art. #130, 5 pages. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i1.130 original research are south african speech-language therapists adequately equipped to assess english additional language (eal) speakers who are from an indigenous linguistic and cultural background? a profile and exploration of the current situation thandeka mdlalo, penelope flack, robin joubert received: 23 may 2015; accepted: 29 nov. 2015; published: 18 mar. 2016 copyright: © 2016. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract this article presents the results of a survey conducted on speech-language therapists (slts) regarding current practices in the assessment of english additional language (eal) speakers in south africa. it forms part of the rationale for a broader (phd) study that critiques the use of assessment instruments on eal speakers from an indigenous linguistic and cultural background. this article discusses an aspect of the broader research and presents the background, method, findings, discussion and implications of the survey. the results of this survey highlight the challenges of human and material resources to, and the dominance of english in, the profession in south africa. the findings contribute to understanding critical factors for acquiring reliable and valid assessment results with diverse populations, particularly the implications from a cultural and linguistic perspective. background to the research survey this article presents an aspect of a larger phd study, the aim of which was to critically evaluate and interrogate the use of language assessment tools, in their current form, within the south african context and to produce guidelines for adaptations to these tools that will better accommodate english additional language (eal) speakers. these guidelines and principles could be used by language professionals to manage the assessment process and interpretation of findings from eal speakers in a more accurate, appropriate and equitable manner. the term ‘eal speaker’ is used here to specifically refer to south african multilinguals who are non-mother tongue speakers of english and are from indigenous language and cultural backgrounds1. the larger study makes use of a specific screening tool as a model, to exemplify and illustrate the argument. in this study, the cultural and linguistic relevance of this commonly used screening tool is interrogated from four different viewpoints: firstly, the perspective of the children, who are the target population of the tool; secondly, that of the parents and community, who play a significant role in the socialisation of the children; thirdly, from the perspective of the academics from an indigenous language and cultural background, who provide an academic perspective of the tool; and, finally, that of speech-language therapist (slt) practitioners who administer the tool and interpret the findings. as language assessments are conducted on people who exist within a cultural context, the cultural capital is embedded in language (peltier, 2010; seidman, 2008; westby, 2009). since the larger study focuses on the relationship between language and culture and adopts an ecological approach to the problems addressed in the study, a conceptual model that encompasses a strong ecological and cultural component was selected; that is taylor’s (1986) cultural framework for viewing normal and pathological communication. as part of this larger study, a national survey was conducted with slts to establish current practices in assessment and intervention, training and challenges experienced when working with a client who is an eal speaker. the discipline of speech language pathology (slp) has a clientele, both in south africa and globally, which is becoming increasingly multilingual and multicultural (jordaan, 2008; williams & mcleod, 2012; wium, 2010). the assessment of diverse populations needs to take into account and to accommodate this diversity of languages and cultures (gopaul-mcnicol & armour-thomas, 2002; mcleod, 2014; tabors, 2008). the provision of a culture-fair assessment, however, presents many challenges to the profession (caesar & kohler, 2007; landsberg, 2005; mcleod, verdon & bowen, 2013; pillay, 2003). these include the limited knowledge and understanding that therapists have of the cultures and language groups from which their clientele may come. knowledge of the language and cultural background of clients is significant for the therapist as it influences the outcome of assessment and interpretation of their findings, especially when the therapist is likely to use their own worldview as a basis for this process. it also aids the slt in discriminating between a language disorder and language difference in eal speakers (n. miller, 1984; thordardottir, 2011). language pathology may include: difficulty in understanding or expression of the meaning of language, problems with understanding or appropriate use of the grammatical or morphological (involving units of meaning) rules, appropriate use in social context and problems with speech sounds, patterns or rules of organisation (shipley & mcafee, 2004). the difficulties described here manifest in whatever language the individual uses. it is thus a language problem and not a second language (l2) difficulty. on the other hand, there may be language differences in the production by the individual, in the process of learning a l2 (i.e. undergoing bilingualism). these differences, which may be perceived as errors by a non-informed ear, will only manifest in the l2 or language being developed rather than the mother tongue. they may be influenced by factors such as language, culture and frame of reference. appropriate training plays a crucial role in facilitating increased linguistic and cultural understanding of the client’s background (du plessis, 2010; higgs, 2010). as these challenges are relevant for the south african context there is a need for research to address the gap in the assessment of our diverse population. this is the rationale for the survey, the outcome of which is discussed in this article. the research method the broader phd study used a mixed methods approach with multiple data collection methods such as a survey, focus groups, individual interviews, test administration and consensus methods. the methodological design was comprised of two phases with the national survey being part of the preparatory phase, in order to set the foundation for and support the rationale for the research. apart from establishing the assessment and therapeutic interventions that slts use with their clientele, the survey also assisted in providing an indication of the profile of the south african slt. a national survey of 1000 slts, registered with the health professions council of south africa (hpcsa), was thus conducted using random sampling. questionnaires with open and closed-ended questions on areas related to employment, clients, caseload, choice of language for practice, current practices in assessment and intervention, training and challenges were sent to slts on the register. firstly, a pilot study was conducted by sending 100 questionnaires to slts (10%) from this national register and the questionnaire was revised, taking into account these responses. based on the responses received, changes were made, such as rephrasing of ambiguous questions on language use and current management and extension of some of the options provided in questions on caseload, employment and experience with eals. the survey questionnaires were then sent to 1000 hpcsa registered slts via the postal service and a 15% (∑150) response rate was achieved. despite the relatively low response rate, the results are consistent with findings of global and local research (jordaan, 2008; von dulm & southwood, 2013), which had similar response rates. data were analysed using the statistical package for social sciences (spss 18). the data were organised into simple frequencies and presented in tables and graphs. results of the survey the results of the survey show that 99% of slts sampled were from english or afrikaans speaking backgrounds and competent in these languages (figure 1). furthermore, 89% of these slts had eal speakers in their caseload and 86% of the slts used english in the assessment of these eals. the results also suggest that english standardised language assessment tools, which have been normed on populations predominantly in the us and uk and are inappropriate for eal speakers, remain the tests most commonly used by slts to assess this population and they are administered in english. figure 1: a bar chart showing the language competence percentages of slts in sa (data to be interpreted with caution due to low response rate). there are several reasons given by the therapists for the choice of english in assessments, but the most common is the therapist’s self-proclaimed restricted competence in other african languages, as reflected in the quotes below: ‘i feel equipped when the goal is to improve english language comprehension and expression’. ‘i only work in english medium schools where i have an understanding of culture or background and thus sensitivity thereof’. ‘i feel equipped because i don’t take on children if therapy is not in english’. ‘my level of competence in understanding, speaking and thinking about all languages (except english) is insufficient in providing quality accountable service’. although some of the therapists are content with the use of english, as reflected in their responses, others expressed some concerns: ‘assessment tools and programmes are foreign to these children (referring to eal speaking children) regarding the language as well as cultural barriers’. ‘there are several reasons why these assessment tools present with these barriers and these include a different language, culture, experience or dialect’. discussion and implications of the results because of the low response rate to the questionnaires, the findings should be viewed with some caution in terms of conclusiveness. they do however provide an interesting insight into the direction of a broader picture of the profile of slts and their test usage in sa. the survey findings suggest that eal speakers are currently mostly being assessed by slts who come from different linguistic and cultural backgrounds to those of the clients (saslha, 2012, 2013; von dulm & southwood, 2013). this information accentuates the issue of the relationship between language and culture. language represents a powerful tool of self-definition and expression and becomes a means through which various cultural and social groups can find unique expression (han & price, 2015, jandt, 2000; ji, zhang & nisbett, 2004; tabors, 2008). it can thus be argued that language is a cultural phenomenon (ball & peltier, 2011; riley, 2007; sardar & van loon, 2004). bearing this in mind, it becomes essential for professionals in the language field to understand and always draw on mother tongue, socio-cultural meanings when involved in the assessment of language of multilingual and multicultural populations (gopaul-mcnicol & armour-thomas, 2002; solarsh & alant, 2006; westby, 2009). in south africa, the majority of eal speakers use an african language as their mother tongue (http://www.statssa.gov.za), and the majority of slts do not. the survey results indicate that eal speakers are thus currently being evaluated by slts who do not speak or understand an african language. in addition, feedback on the questionnaire also reveals that slts have restricted understanding of the cultures linked to these languages. it can thus be assumed that the slt will use their own linguistic and cultural background as a frame of reference for interpreting the assessment results and this in turn may further influence the language choice and use in assessment as well as interpretation of results. the picture thus emerging from the results of the survey places the slt in a very powerful position as they can attach their own cultural and linguistic worldview to the meaning of the assessment and criteria for success. this powerful position further raises the question as to whether the meaning that the slt attaches to their assessment, serves the interests of justice and equality as they relate to the client. issues of justice and equality underpin current discussions within the profession pertaining to hegemonic discourses, language and practices that reproduce them (kathard & pillay, 2013, 2015). the nature and number of comments from some of the respondents suggest that they concur that the current situation is not ideal and indicate the need for a greater research effort into the creation of more culturally and linguistically relevant language assessment materials for eal speakers in south africa. as a result of the challenges mentioned by the slts in the survey, specifically a paucity of culturally and linguistically relevant tests developed for the south african population (naudé, louw & weideman, 2007; pascoe & norman, 2011), most evaluations are conducted using tests developed and normed on populations that are predominantly from the us or uk. that these tests are not ideal in their current form for assessing a south african eal speaker who is from an indigenous language and cultural background goes without saying and this is confirmed by research into language development and assessment (caesar & kohler, 2007; gopaul-mcnicol & armour-thomas, 2002; moro, 2008; pierce & williams, 2013). tests currently used by slts in south africa are predominantly based on a linguistic, cultural and social context that is largely european and american. the guidelines for the administration of these tests do not refer to the south african multilingual and multicultural population. the test developers thus do not expect them to be used on populations outside of those stipulated in the test. one of the reasons that the slts provided for mostly conducting the assessment and therapy of eal speakers in english is the demand for the use of english by the parents of the eal speaking children. these demands are linked to the perception by parents of eal children that english is the language linked to progress in education. despite the recognition of 11 official languages in sa, english remains the dominant language in all sectors of society (burger, 2011; green, 2008; kamwangamalu, 2000; landsberg, 2005; muendane, 2006), including education (republic of south africa, 2011). many african eal speaking children from an indigenous language and cultural background are thus taught in settings where english is the medium of instruction (moi). the language of learning and teaching (lolt) in many south african schools is english (lafon, 2007; landsberg, 2005). many slts therefore try to justify their persistence in assessment and intervention in english even for eal speakers, the evidence of which is clear in the survey results, which showed that 86% of the slts used english in language assessments and therapy. the results of this survey are particularly crucial, because of the many eal speakers from indigenous cultural and linguistic backgrounds in south africa who are in schools where english is the moi and who are referred by teachers for language assessments to slts. even though the curriculum may be presented in english, this is not the mother tongue of these south african children, nor is their cultural background reflected (higgs, 2010; landsberg, 2005; ntuli, 2002). african language and culture influences the knowledge and belief system of the eal speaker (lemmer, meier & van wyk, 2006; metz & gaie, 2010) and therefore influences their response to the tests, which have been predominantly normed on us or uk populations, societies whose language and culture is different from theirs (higgs, 2003; kroes, 2005; makgoba, 1999; semali, 1999). therefore, the background, values and stories of the eal speaker, such as found in traditional oral african culture manifesting in african signs and symbols (maathai, 2009; mutwa, 1998), tend to be disregarded, devalued, ignored or only superficially addressed (d. miller, 2012) when resources for evaluations do not reflect the african experience. as the majority of eal speakers in south africa are african language mother tongue speakers, it can be said that the child is treated as a tabula rasa when the african worldview (behrens, 2010; mucina 2013; ntuli, 1999, 2002) they bring is ignored. the children’s choice, based on their frame of reference, is excluded as an option in the assessment tool scoring system. the outcome may likely thus create a distorted reflection of the language ability of an eal speaker from an indigenous language and cultural background and the slt may thus, unintentionally, pathologise a child who presents with a language difference. conclusion the results of the study indicate that the average slt assessing an eal child is still predominantly either an english or afrikaans speaking woman, who is not competent in an african language. saville-troike (1986, p. 48) maintains that ‘whether we realise it or not, each of us sees the world from a culturally conditioned perspective that we share with the other members of the group’. thus, the frame of reference of these slts is based upon their own socio-cultural background, which in turn influences their interpretation of the child’s response. these findings have implications for the selection and training of slts in south africa and suggest a direction for postgraduate research in this discipline. the current recruitment of african language speakers for training in the profession needs to be intensified to accommodate the assessment and management of this population. sections of the speech-language pathology curriculum pertaining to bilingualism and cultural and linguistic diversity need to be enhanced for more effective preparation of the slts who will work with eal speakers. in addition, postgraduate research that addresses these discrepancies should be encouraged. although it is necessary that further research be conducted to create more culturally and linguistically relevant tools for the eal population, the practising slts remain accountable for accessing research-based evidence on the assessment and management of the eal population. failure to do so constitutes a contravention of the profession’s ethical code of conduct. it is hoped that the outcome of this research will create greater sensitivity in the application of non-standardised language screening tests to eal speakers in this country. acknowledgements competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions this article is based on a phd study conducted by t.m. (speech therapist, durban), who was supervised by p.f. (university of kwazulu-natal) and r.j (university of kwazulu-natal). the paper was drafted by t.m. and p.f. and r.j. contributed to all sections of the final article. references ball, s., & peltier, j. (2011). cultural safety, relevance and effectiveness of speech and language services to indigenous children. paper presented at the caslpa 2011 conference, montreal, canada, 27–30 april 2011. behrens, k. (2010). exploring african holism with respect to the environment. environmental values, 19(4), 465–484. http://dx.doi.org/10.3197/096327110x531561 burger, d. (2011). south africa year book 2010/11. (18th edn.). johannesburg, south africa: gcis. caesar, l.g., & kohler, p.d. (2007). the state of school based bilingual assessment: actual practice versus recommended guidelines. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 38, 190–200. http://dx.doi.org/10.1044/0161-1461(2007/020) du plessis, s. (2010). redefining the role of the speech-language therapist in the educational context: overcoming the barriers. paper presented at the new perspectives in our profession conference, pretoria, south africa, 19–20 april 2010. gopaul-mcnicol, s., & armour-thomas, e. (2002). assessment and culture: psychological tests with minority populations. san diego, ca: academic press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/b978-012290451-6/50011-8 green, p. (2008). choice not fate: the life and times of trevor manuel. johannesburg, south africa: penguin. han, e.j., & price, p.g. (2015). uncovering the hidden power of language: critical race theory, critical language socialization and multicultural families in korea. journal of intercultural communication, 44(2),108–131. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/17475759.2015.1028427 higgs, p. (2003). african philosophy and the transformation of educational discourse in south africa. journal of education, 30, 5–22. higgs, p. (2010). towards an indigenous african epistemology of community in education research. procedia social and behavioral sciences, 2(2), 2414–2421. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.sbspro.2010.03.347 jandt, f. (2000). an introduction to intercultural communication: identities in a global community. (4th edn.). london, uk: sage. ji, l., zhang, z., & nisbett, r. (2004). is it culture or is it language? examination of language effects in cross-cultural research on categorisation. journal of personality and social language, 87(1), 57–65. http://dx.doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.87.1.57 jordaan, h. (2008). clinical intervention for bilingual children. folia phoniatrica et logopaedica, 60, 97–105. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000114652 kamwangamalu, n.m. (2000). a new language policy, old language practices: status planning for african languages in multilingual south africa. south african journal of african languages, 20(1), 50–65. kathard, h., & pillay, m. (2013). promoting change through political consciousness: a south african speech-language pathology response to the world report on disability. international journal of speech-language pathology, 15(1), 84–89. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2012.757803 kathard, h., & pillay, m. (2015). decolonising health professionals’ education: audiology and speech therapy in south africa. african journal of rhetoric: transitions and transformative rhetoric, 17, 193–227. kroes, h. (2005). the language perspective. in c.j. russo, j. beckmann, & j. jansen (eds.), equal educational opportunities (pp. 236–257). pretoria, south africa: van schaik. lafon, m. (2007). asikhulume! african languages for all, a powerful strategy for spearheading transformation and improvement of the south african education system. paper presented at the the standardisation of african languages: language political realities, university of pretoria, pretoria, south africa, 29 march 2007. landsberg, e. (2005). addressing barriers to learning: a south african perspective. pretoria, south africa: van schaik. lemmer, e.m., meier, c., & van wyk, j.n. (2006). multicultural education: an educator’s manual. pretoria, south africa: van schaik. maathai, w. (2009). the challenge for africa – a new vision. london, uk: william heineman. makgoba, m.w. (ed.). (1999). african renaissance. cape town, south africa: mafube, tafelberg. mcleod, s. (2014). resourcing speech-language pathologists to work with multilingual children. international journal of speech-language pathology, 16(3), 208–218. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2013.876666 mcleod, s., verdon, s., & bowen, c. (2013). international aspirations for speech-language pathologists’ practice with multilingual children with speech sound disorders: development of a position paper. journal of communication disorders, 46(4), 375–387. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2013.04.003 metz, t., & gaie, j.b.r. (2010). the african ethic of ubuntu/botho: implications for research on morality. journal of moral education: special issue: moral education in sub-saharan africa—culture, economics, conflict and aids, 39(3), 273–290. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/03057240.2010.497609 miller, d. (2012). african-centered thought on western objectification: autoethnographic reflections. paper presented at the international congress of qualitative inquiry, university of illinois at urbana-champaign, il, 16–19 may 2012. miller, n. (1984). bilingualism and language disability: assessment and remediation. san diego, ca: college-hill press. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/978-1-4899-7238-5 moro, m.r. (2008). transcultural issues for children and parents in a changing world. paper presented at the 18th world congress for the international association for child and adolescent psychiatry and allied professions, istanbul, turkey, 27 april–03 may 2008. mucina, d.d. (2013). ubuntu orality as a living philosophy. the journal of pan african studies, 6(4), 18–35. muendane, m.n. (2006). i am an african: embrace your identity, escape victimisation. pretoria, south africa: soultalk. mutwa, c. (1998). indaba, my children. edinburgh, scotland: payback press. naudé, e., louw, b., & weideman, a. (2007). first steps toward tools for language assessment in multilingual preschoolers. south african linguistics applied language studies, 25(4), 519–538. http://dx.doi.org/10.2989/16073610709486479 ntuli, p. (1999). the missing link between culture and education: are we still chasing gods that are not our own? in m.w. makgoba (ed.), african renaissance (pp. 184–199). cape town, south africa: mafube, tafelberg. ntuli, p. (2002). indigenous knowledge systems and the african renaissance – laying a foundation for the creation of counter-hegemonic discourses. in c. hoppers (ed.), indigenous knowledge and the integration of knowledge systems (pp. 53–66). claremont, south africa: new africa books. pascoe, m., & norman, v. (2011). contextually relevant resources in speech-language therapy and audiology in south africa: are there any? south african journal of communication disorders, 58(1), 2–5. peltier, s. (2010). facilitating language and literacy learning for students with aboriginal english dialects. canadian journal of native education, suppl. aboriginal englishes and education. pierce, w.m., & williams, c. (2013). the cultural appropriateness and diagnostic usefulness of standardized language assessments for indigenous australian children. international journal of speech-language pathology, 15(4), 429–440. pillay, m. (2003). cross-cultural practice: what is it really about. folio phoniatrica et logogopaedia, 55(6), 293–299. http://dx.doi.org/10.1159/000073252 republic of south africa. (2011). curriculum and assessment policy statement. pretoria, south africa: government printers. riley, p. (2007). language, culture and identity. london, uk: continuum. sardar, z., & van loon, b. (2004). introducing cultural studies. royston, uk: icon books. saslha. (2012). national speech therapy/audiology forum, free state, south africa, 27–29 august. saslha. (2013). national speech therapy/audiology forum, gauteng, south africa, 22–24 october. saville-troike, m. (1986). anthropological considerations in study of communication. in o. taylor (ed.), nature of communication disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse populations (pp. 44–72). san diego, ca: college-hill press. seidman, s. (2008). contested knowledge: social theory today (4th edn.). victoria, australia: blackwell. semali, l. (1999). community as classroom: dilemmas of valuing african indigenous literacy in education. international review of education, 45(3–4), 305–319. http://dx.doi.org/10.1023/a:1003859023590 shipley, k.g., & mcafee, j.g. (2004). assessment in speech-language pathology: a resource manual. (3rd edn.). london, uk: thomson delmar learning. solarsh, b., & alant, e. (2006). the challenge of cross-cultural assessment – the test of ability to explain for zulu -speaking children. journal of communication disorders, 39(2), 109–138. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jcomdis.2005.11.002 tabors, p. (2008). one child, two languages. usa: brookes publishing. taylor, o. (1986). the nature of communication disorders in culturally and linguistically diverse populations. san diego, ca: college-hill press. thordardottir, e. (2011). bilingualism: normal development and clinical intervention. paper presented at the caslpa 2011 conference, montreal, canada, 27–30 april 2011. von dulm, o., & southwood, f. (2013). child language assessment and intervention in multilingual and multicultural south africa: findings of a national survey. stellenbosch papers in linguistics, 42, 55–76. westby, c. (2009). considerations in working successfully with culturally/linguistically diverse families in assessment and intervention of communication disorders. seminars in speech and language, 30(4), 279–289. http://dx.doi.org/10.1055/s-0029-1241725 williams, c., & mcleod, s. (2012). speech-language pathologists’ assessment and intervention practices with multilingual children. international journal of speech –language pathology, 14(3), 292–305. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/17549507.2011.636071 wium, a.m. (2010). speech-language therapists in previously disadvantaged schools: factors affecting support. paper presented at the saslha perspectives on our professions conference, pretoria, south africa, 19–20 april 2010. footnote 1. not a homogenous group there may be interand intra-linguistic and cultural variability abstract introduction intervening early with dysphagia the need for a contextually relevant and valid dysphagia screening tool methodology the screening tool (refer to appendix 1) results of main study validity using cohen’s kappa discussion conclusion acknowledgements references appendix 1 about the author(s) calli ostrofsky department of speech and hearing therapy, university of witwatersrand, south africa jaishika seedat department of speech and hearing therapy, university of witwatersrand, south africa citation ostrofsky, c., & seedat, j. (2016). the south african dysphagia screening tool (sads): a screening tool for a developing context. south african journal of communication disorders 63(1), art. #117, 9 pages. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i1.117 original research the south african dysphagia screening tool (sads): a screening tool for a developing context calli ostrofsky, jaishika seedat received: 28 jan. 2014; accepted: 27 apr. 2015; published: 16 feb. 2016 copyright: © 2016. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract background: notwithstanding its value, there are challenges and limitations to implementing a dysphagia screening tool from a developed contexts in a developing context. the need for a reliable and valid screening tool for dysphagia that considers context, systemic rules and resources was identified to prevent further medical compromise, optimise dysphagia prognosis and ultimately hasten patients’ return to home or work. methodology: to establish the validity and reliability of the south african dysphagia screening tool (sads) for acute stroke patients accessing government hospital services. the study was a quantitative, non-experimental, correlational cross-sectional design with a retrospective component. convenient sampling was used to recruit 18 speech-language therapists and 63 acute stroke patients from three south african government hospitals. the sads consists of 20 test items and was administered by speech-language therapists. screening was followed by a diagnostic dysphagia assessment. the administrator of the tool was not involved in completing the diagnostic assessment, to eliminate bias and prevent contamination of results from screener to diagnostic assessment. sensitivity, validity and efficacy of the screening tool were evaluated against the results of the diagnostic dysphagia assessment. cohen’s kappa measures determined inter-rater agreement between the results of the sads and the diagnostic assessment. results and conclusion: the sads was proven to be valid and reliable. cohen’s kappa indicated a high inter-rater reliability and showed high sensitivity and adequate specificity in detecting dysphagia amongst acute stroke patients who were at risk for dysphagia. the sads was characterised by concurrent, content and face validity. as a first step in establishing contextual appropriateness, the sads is a valid and reliable screening tool that is sensitive in identifying stroke patients at risk for dysphagia within government hospitals in south africa. introduction there are many challenges surrounding effective identification of patients who present with dysphagia within government hospitals in developing contexts. some of these challenges include timing of identification of a swallowing difficulty, timing of the assessment and use of non-standardised contextualised protocols for assessment and intervention (seedat, 2013). the lack of standardisation contributes to variable service delivery from one patient to the next. this is problematic. for decades, speech-language therapists (slts) from less developed contexts have relied on internationally developed tools to guide assessment and intervention in dysphagia, as these had a research and evidence-based underpinning (emanuel, wendler, killen & grady, 2004). two key factors have necessitated a revision of this almost standard protocol of merely implementing internationally developed tools on the south african population. firstly with disease profiles, unemployment rates, dietary intake, accessibility of services and socio-economic status changing dramatically over the last two decades within countries across the world, it is becoming increasingly difficult to compare patient profiles from developed contexts with those from less developed and developing contexts (mamdani, 2011). direct application and use of guidelines without any tailoring or modification is becoming increasingly inappropriate and inadequate to meet the needs of the clients from less developed contexts. linked to this is the status of government health care institutions in developing contexts. in south africa, financial restrictions and budgets dictate what equipment, if any, hospitals have (cullinan, 2006). challenges around availability of equipment, malfunctioning equipment, stolen equipment and servicing of equipment remain significant obstacles with dysphagia service provision in government hospitals. having necessary objective measures in dysphagia intervention such as videofluoroscopy is considered a luxury, with even basic consumables, such as mouth care kits for oral hygiene, often unavailable. secondly, whilst knowledge and awareness around swallowing and swallowing impairments have improved, this has an implication for the workloads of slts employed at hospitals. regardless of its status as a priority for hospital-based slts, dysphagia is but one amongst an array of other speech therapy services patients at hospitals require (american speech-language and hearing association, 2013; health professions council of south africa, 1988; south african speech-language and hearing association, 2009). consequently time, human resources and efficiency of services are compromised for the increasing number of patients that need to be seen who present with dysphagia as well as other speech and language communication disorders. this has also been influenced by the increasing prevalence of chronic disorders such as strokes and hiv, cancer (head and neck) and degenerative neurologic conditions as a result of lifestyle changes, eating habits and poor medical follow-up (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010; brainin, teuschl & kalra, 2007; connor et al., 2008; crary et al., 2013). other reasons limiting direct implementation of internationally developed tools pertain to institutional ‘culture’ amongst health care institutions in south africa. multidisciplinary team management is not ideal (martens, cameron & simonsen, 1990; seedat, 2013). hence, it remains challenging to rely on other health professionals as part of team management due to poor role clarity and limited knowledge of each other’s role and responsibilities when working with the patient with dysphagia (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010). a need for a contextualised, specific, valid and reliable dysphagia screening tool that could address some of the aforementioned barriers was identified. intervening early with dysphagia dysphagia, characterised by difficulty with the passage of food from the mouth to the stomach, is defined as a swallowing disorder affecting the oral, pharyngeal or oesophageal phase of swallowing (falsetti et al., 2009). dysphagia is commonly a symptom of neurological disease such as stroke (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010). the association between stroke and dysphagia has been established, with reports ranging from one-third to two-thirds of acute stroke patients presenting with dysphagia (perry, 2000). whilst exact statistics on the incidence of dysphagia amongst stroke patients in south africa are not readily available, one may surmise that given the increasing prevalence of strokes (connor & bryer, 2006; connor et al., 2008), there is likely a consequent increase in associated dysphagia. early evaluation of dysphagia amongst stroke patients may decrease one’s vulnerability to co-morbidities (hinchey et al., 2005), such as aspiration pneumonia, malnutrition, dehydration, airway obstruction or even death (perry, 2000). additional considerations for the patient accessing a government hospital in south africa are (1) financial implications – patients who are breadwinners of the family need to return to work and increased length of hospitalisation impacts this – and (2) an increased likelihood of the patient acquiring other hospital-acquired infections and co-morbidities. early detection facilitates optimal management, minimises occurrence of dysphagia-related complications, as well as other co-morbidities, and can improve the prognosis for dysphagia (blackwell & littlejohns, 2010; heckert, komaroff, adler & barrett, 2009; marik & kaplan, 2003; martino, pron & diamant, 2000). cost efficiency for the hospital department, for the hospital and for the patient are important considerations in any resource-constrained and developing context. the need for a contextually relevant and valid dysphagia screening tool a standardised screening tool facilitates identification of a disorder (martino et al., 2000). many screening tools for dysphagia with established reliability and validity exist (martino et al., 2009; trapl et al., 2007). mamdani (2011), however, cautions against readily accepting and implementing tools developed in first world countries to populations in developing second and third world countries. as noted above, populations, environments, systems and so on may be very different, necessitating modification of internationally developed tools (mamdani, 2011). it is important that a screening tool considers context-specific variables, resources, logistics and systemic rules. the high demand on health care professionals and the government health care system itself increases the likelihood of subtle problems, further medical compromise and delayed identification of dysphagia only when the patient is at a critical stage or when the dysphagia becomes more ‘overt’ to the casual observer. the availability and use of a context-appropriate screening tool can relieve the demand on staff, resources and time through its simplicity, whilst maximising dysphagia detection. as it stands, implications for early, reliable and efficient dysphagia identification and management within this context, given the challenges, are discouraging. competency, knowledge and commitment of the administrator of the screening tool must be given consideration in the development of a contextually relevant and appropriate tool. optimal features of a screening tool are presented in table 1. table 1: guidelines to adhere to when developing a screening tool. problem statement the availability of numerous screening tools, each validated and proven reliable, raised the question of the need to develop yet another screening tool. the dilemma of which existing screening tool to validate on the south african population led to the development of a new screening tool. the new tool incorporated the proven benefits of existing tools whilst heeding their limitations and simultaneously ensuring cultural and linguistic sensitivity for the general south african population. it also considered the realities of acute hospital wards, the staff available, time available and access to resources. however, the question remained: would the newly developed dysphagia screening tool facilitate valid and reliable identification of dysphagia, amongst patients presenting with stroke as their underlying medical pathology? methodology aim to assess the reliability and validity of a newly developed dysphagia screening tool to identify dysphagia in acute adult patients presenting with stroke. design a quantitative research methodology using a non-experimental, correlational cross-sectional design was used. a retrospective component was necessary to review patient files after administration of a diagnostic dysphagia assessment. this is discussed below. process and sample three government hospitals in south africa were the research sites. necessary ethical approval was obtained from the university of witwatersrand human research and ethics committee (medical; protocol no. m120215). there were two cohorts of participants: 18 slts and 63 stroke patients. convenience sampling was used to recruit both cohorts. the participants with dysphagia were recruited by the recruited slt participants. the patient sample adequately represented patients attending government hospitals in south africa from varying financial, sociolinguistic and cultural backgrounds. the patient sample received the sads (dysphagia screening) and following this, within 24 hours, a diagnostic dysphagia assessment. 63 patients were screened and 62 received a diagnostic dysphagia assessment. the slt sample were clinicians working within the government hospitals from which the patient sample was recruited. the clinician sample was responsible for conducting the sads and the diagnostic dysphagia assessment. although they may be regarded as research assistants, they have been described as a participant sample, as they had to adhere to inclusion and exclusion criteria and be recruited by employing convenience sampling. further, it was necessary for the slts to consent to participate in the study. data collection figure 1 provides an illustration of the procedure that was followed for data collection. figure 1: process followed for data collection the pilot study evaluated aspects of the design and procedure of the research, as well as the developed research tool in terms of feasibility, usefulness and ease of administration (clark-carter, 2010; mcburney & white, 2010). the screening tool (refer to appendix 1) the south african dysphagia screening (sads) tool consists of four sections and 20 test items. the items within each subsection are described below. section 1 the items in section 1 were aimed at determining the alertness of the stroke participant. a patient’s compromised state of consciousness may have implications on their ability to swallow safely or alert the slt to possible swallowing difficulty that is the patient is experiencing (martino et al., 2009). section 2 items in this section provide subjective interpretations of the screening results. regardless of the answer, items in section 2 do not categorise the patient as a ‘refer’ but need to be considered. the first item in section 2 evaluates the patient’s position. according to tanner (2007), swallowing whilst seated in an upright position achieves maximum protection of the airway and reduces the chances of aspiration. thus, incorrect positioning may compromise airway protection. the second item in section 2 requires the patient to count. this item serves a two-fold function. it allows the slt to gain an impression of the patient’s voice, as well as insight into the patient’s receptive language understanding. assessment of swallowing requires patients to follow instructions (e.g. open your mouth), be alert and have a degree of understanding that will enable an appropriate assessment. inability to understand simple instructions may have implications on ability to swallow safely or alert the slt to possible swallowing difficulties that the patient may be experiencing (martino et al., 2009). voicing provides information on laryngeal functioning (cichero & murdoch, 2006). careful attention is to be directed to the patient’s voice quality prior to and after each delivery of food or liquid in order to ascertain whether food is entering the larynx, thus potentially leading to aspiration (murray, 2000). if a patient is unable to produce voicing when they receptively understand the instruction, this may be indicative of possible respiratory problems, vocal fold involvement or laryngeal weakness (cichero & murdoch, 2006). the third item of section 2 evaluates the patient’s ability to perform a volitional dry swallow. a lack of rapid and forceful elevation of the larynx during a dry swallow as well as lack of observed palpitation of the neck during a dry swallow is an indicator of possible increased risk of a swallowing difficulty (perlman & schulze-delrieu, 1998). section 3 the items in section 3 determine the oral motor skills of the stroke participant. the behaviours or functions, as well as structures of the oral mechanism need to be carefully observed during the oral motor exam, as these affect the patient’s ability to chew or swallow safely (groher, 1997; shipley & mcafee, 2004). deficits may affect the oral preparatory phase of swallowing, in terms of poor oral control because of lip and tongue weakness, lingual weakness, reduced range of motion and in-coordination, with bolus formation, manipulation, chewing and swallowing being affected (langmore, 2001; shipley & mcafee, 2004). the patient’s ability to cough voluntarily needs to be determined as there is an increased risk for aspiration in patients who have a weakened voluntary cough (smith-hammond et al., 2001). observation of facial asymmetry and lip symmetry must be included as facial weakness commonly occurs after a stroke, most typically in the lower facial muscles (geyer, gomez, sheppard & akhtar, 2009). section 4 this section involves the food trial. the patient must be presented with small amounts (5 ml) of food of different viscosity. presentation is to progress from consistencies that are the easiest for the stroke patient to manage to the most difficult for the patient to manage (shipley & mcafee, 2004). thus, pureed foods (e.g. banana beaten with a fork and mixed with water to form a puree consistency, yogurt and mageu, a traditional yogurt drink), followed by a soft solid (e.g. mash potatoes, pap, which is similar to mash, and boiled vegetables) and, lastly, a liquid (i.e. water). pureed foods easily form a bolus and chewing is not required. with a soft solid, chewing is required for bolus formation. liquids, however, are the most difficult to manage as they do not form a bolus and swallowing requires the least amount of voluntary and reflexive control, increasing the possibility of aspiration (shipley & mcafee, 2004). furthermore, unless oral care is good in the patient being assessed, choking on the water will place the patient at risk for aspiration and aspiration pneumonia as a result of bacteria from the oral cavity. section 4 includes observable signs that can allude to dysphagia when swallowing. these include food spillage, food pocketing, coughing after swallowing and a delayed, absent or painful swallow (shipley & mcafee, 2004). test administration the administration of the sads involves four subsections as noted above. for each item, the administering slt is required to indicate ‘yes’ or ‘no’ based on the patient’s performance on an item. each subsection requires either a ‘yes’ or ‘no’ response. difficulty with any item would result in termination of the screening and the patient is referred for further diagnostic evaluation. pass and fail criteria the test administrator has the responsibility to pass or fail the patient. items in section 2 are items that are subjective descriptions and are therefore not referral criteria. a fail on any of the items in section 1, 3 or 4 of the screening tool is indicative of risk of dysphagia, thus a positive result would be indicated on the screening tool. if a fail is indicated in section 3, the screening is to be discontinued. within section 4, if a fail is indicated in subsection a, the screening is to be discontinued. if a fail is indicated in section 4, subsection b, the screening is to be discontinued. the sads was designed to be simple and quick to administer. ease of administration and interpretation were vital prerequisites in the development of the sads. as part of the study protocol to establish reliability and validity, the results of the sads were correlated with a subsequent diagnostic dysphagia assessment. the pilot study the pilot study was conducted at a government hospital different to those used in the main study. upon completion of the screening and diagnostic assessments, the respective slts were required to complete a questionnaire probing the content of the sads. the responses allowed the researcher to determine the need for items to be added, omitted or modified on the screening tool. based on the results of the pilot study and the questionnaire, the following adjustments were made: modification of the item relating to receptive ability of patient. if the patient presented with poor receptive understanding, the screening was continued and the patient was not referred based on that criterion. this patient was to be assessed with caution. patient position: the patient needs to be positioned with caution, and upright positioning was not a referral criterion. addition of item: volitional swallow. results of main study there were 63 participants who received the sads. however, only 62 participants underwent the diagnostic assessment. hence, the calculations for the screening versus the diagnostic assessment reflect these numbers. all further calculations take this missing frequency into consideration. from the sample, 30% (n = 19) of the participants passed the screening with the sads, and 69.84% (n = 44) were referred from the screening (see table 2). one stroke participant who was screened was unavailable at the time of the diagnostic assessment and was not followed up. hence, only 62 stroke participants received the diagnostic dysphagia assessment, results of which are seen in table 3. table 2: screening results from the sads (n = 63) table 3: assessment results from diagnostic dysphagia assessment (n = 62). results of the diagnostic assessment indicated that 52.23% (n = 33) of the participants presented with dysphagia and 46.77% (n = 29) of the participants did not present with dysphagia. reliability using cohen’s kappa the evaluation of the relationship between the dysphagia screening tool and the diagnostic dysphagia assessment battery was done using correlation coefficients (schiavetti & metz, 2002). cohen’s kappa was used to determine the agreement between two dichotomous variables (wood, 2007). using cohen’s kappa, measures of positive agreement and negative agreement provided information regarding the types of agreement that presented between the screening tool and the diagnostic assessment. the results are presented in table 4. table 4: results of cohen’s kappa the percentage of agreement refers to the agreement between the results of the sads and the diagnostic assessment (wood, 2007). thus, 80.64% of the screenings elicited the correct results (i.e. correct pass and referrals). however, the calculation of percentage of agreement has been critiqued as not being an adequate measure of inter-rater reliability. thus, cohen’s kappa, which is a preferred measure of inter-rater reliability, as opposed to percentage agreement, incorporates a calculation of hypothetical probability of chance agreements (wood, 2007). thus, the probability that stroke participants presented with dysphagia was calculated. the probability that the participants were referred was also calculated. the calculations of probability were then used to calculate cohen’s kappa. the probability that participants were referred based on the screening and presented with dysphagia plus the probability that a patient passed the screening and did not have dysphagia (wood, 2007) was calculated to be 0.51. probability for chance agreement refers to the probability that the participant had dysphagia as well as the probability that the participant was referred by the screening and did present with dysphagia was 0.60. the correlation coefficient for kappa can range from –1.0 to + 1.0; a kappa of 1.0 is indicative of perfect agreement and a kappa of zero shows a poor correlation between the two variables (wood, 2007). according to wood (2007), for the purpose of medical studies and diagnosis, a kappa that lies between 0.40 and 0.70 indicates an appropriate inter-rater reliability; the values calculated for this study were thus appropriate, as can be seen in both the percentage of agreement and probability of chance agreement. validity using cohen’s kappa validity is the ability of a test to measure what it is intended to measure (rust & golombok, 1999). content validity (how accurately the questions used in the assessment tool, i.e. the sads, tap into what is being asked without the response being influenced by other variables), face validity (the degree to which an assessment measures what it appears to measure) and concurrent validity (how well the results of one assessment correlate with another assessment designed to measure the same thing, i.e. the sads and the diagnostic dysphagia assessment) were calculated (rust & golombok, 1999). concurrent validity relies on timing; hence, the sads and the diagnostic dysphagia assessment were conducted within 24 hours of each other. the following measures were used in combination to optimise content validity of the sads: clinical judgements and input from experienced slts working in the area of adult dysphagia were incorporated in the initial conceptualisation of the tool (schrock & coscarelli, 2007). these slts were not included as part of the sample of slts recruited into the study. a review of existing dysphagia screening tools in terms of content, as well as each tool’s evidence base, was central to the content of the developed screening tool. each question in the sads directly assessed a particular objective (i.e. a sign or symptom of dysphagia), allowing direct evaluation of an objective. feedback from the slt participants confirmed that reading level and use of vocabulary within the sads were appropriate to enable any health care professional working in an acute health care context to conduct the screening. technical difficulties with instructions and ambiguity of instructions were also considered during the development and construction of the screening tool and were addressed in the pilot study. this did not arise as a concern during the main study. content under-representation, which may interfere with content validity, was avoided by ensuring that the items were adequately able to identify swallowing abnormality and risk of aspiration without indicating severity of dysphagia. the slt participants confirmed the overall face validity and appropriateness of the sads. the percentage of agreement referred to in table 4 was 80.64%. thus, concurrent validity of the sads revealed that it correlated very well with the longer diagnostic assessment conducted. the cohen’s kappa was used to establish this form of validity. sensitivity and specificity a measure of sensitivity and specificity of the sads was calculated. the measures of sensitivity and specificity are binary classification statistical measures. sensitivity measures the proportion of true positives, which are correctly identified as such (i.e. the percentage of dysphagic patients who are correctly identified as having dysphagia). specificity measures the proportion of true negatives which are correctly identified (i.e. the percentage of patients with normal swallowing who are correctly identified as not having dysphagia; haynes, smith & hunsley, 2011). the results were 96.97% sensitivity and 62.07% specificity, as can be seen in table 5. the results showed the sads to be sensitive in detecting patients at risk for dysphagia. the specificity of the sads is lower than its sensitivity; however, the sads was still deemed to be adequate in identifying patients not at risk for dysphagia, when they do not present with the disorder. the specificity calculation indicates that participants were unnecessarily referred as being at risk for dysphagia when, in fact, they did not present with the disorder. table 5: sensitivity and specificity of the sads referral items there were particular items that were more sensitive in detecting signs and symptoms of dysphagia than others. the items in table 6 were the most common referral items in the sads in order of frequency. table 6: most common referral items and percentage of participants that were referred. discussion analysis of the results suggested that the sads was a valid screening tool for dysphagia, with high inter-rater reliability. it was specific in being able to detect stroke patients who present with dysphagia. the percentage of agreement score, 80.64%, suggested high concurrent validity, showing that the results from the sads correlated substantially with the independent diagnostic assessment to which it was theoretically related (frick, barry & kamphaus, 2009). results that were described showed that minimal and minor modifications were necessary to improve overall content validity of the sads, which was achieved. face validity was also acknowledged by the various administrators of the tool, who were qualified clinicians working in adult dysphagia and knowledgeable of contextual challenges and facilitators. a cohen’s kappa value of 0.6 deemed the sads as having appropriate inter-rater reliability (0.40–0.70 suggests good inter-rater reliability for medical studies; wood, 2007). the sensitivity of the sads in detecting risk for dysphagia with the chosen population of patients was 96.96%, suggesting high sensitivity (abramson & abramson, 2008). specificity was slightly lower at 62.06%, but, according to logemann, veis and colangelo (1999) that it still fell in the range of 50%–80% meant that it was adequate. the study showed a sensitivity and specificity pattern of high sensitivity and lower specificity (logemann et al., 1999). there was one false-negative, which was concerning from a dysphagia perspective in terms of complications that could occur from late identification (heckert et al., 2009). it is likely that given the acuteness of the screening (within 24 hours of admission), dysphagia may not have been initially present, as heckert et al. (2009) also found. it is also possible, that further medical deterioration may have occurred subsequent to the screening (heckert et al., 2009). protocols to prevent such omissions need to be considered. from a time perspective, it was confirmed that the sads took 10 minutes or less to complete. it was deemed resource conservative and could be easily administered. whilst the feasibility of the sads for the current context was not the goal, establishing the validity and reliability empirically was the first step in working toward feasibility. the study was able to establish this. conclusion the absence of standardised diagnostic dysphagia assessment protocols across government hospitals in south africa meant that the researchers had to rely on the fact that the protocols used at each hospital in the study were thorough and comprehensive in correctly diagnosing dysphagia in the stroke participants. this was acknowledged as a limitation of the study. an implication of the study is to establish the feasibility of using the sads in an acute government hospital in south africa. whilst different variables were considered in the development of the sads to facilitate contextual feasibility, this was not an aim of the study. there is therefore a need to establish the exact effects of the context in the implementation of the tool, as the validity, reliability and sensitivity of a measure cannot exist independent of the context. acknowledgements competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions c.o. (university of the witwatersrand) was the project leader and j.s. (university of the witwatersrand) was the supervisor of the project. c.o. conducted the study and j.s. made conceptual contributions and provided guidance. c.o and j.s. wrote the manuscript. references abramson, j.h., & abramson, z.h. (2008). research methods in community medicine: surveys, epidemiological research, programme evaluation, clinical trials. (6th edn.). chichester, uk: john wiley & sons ltd. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/9780470987230 american speech-language and hearing association (asha). (2013). definitions of professions. retrieved december 2013, from http://www.asha.org/div40/definitions.htm blackwell, z., & littlejohns, p. (2010). a review of the management of dysphagia: a south african perspective. the journal of neuroscience nursing, 42, 1–9. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/jnn.0b013e3181ce5c86 brainin, m., teuschl, y., & kalra, l. (2007). acute treatment and long-term management of stroke in developing countries. the lancet neurology, 6(6), 553–561. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/s1474-4422(07)70005-4 cichero, j.a., & murdoch, b.e. (2006). dysphagia: foundation, theory and practice. new york, ny: wiley. clark-carter, d. (2010). quantitative psychological research: the complete students companion. (3rd edn.). new york, ny: taylor & francis. connor, m., & bryer, a. (2006). stroke in south africa. cape town, south africa: south african medical research council. connor, m., rheeder, p., bryer, a., meredith, m., dubb, a., & fritz, v. (2008). the south african stroke risk in general practice study. south african medical journal, 95, 334–339. crary, m.a., humphrey, j.l., carnaby-mann, g., sambandam, r., miller, l., & silliman, s. (2013). dysphagia, nutrition, and hydration in ischemic stroke patients at admission and discharge from acute care. dysphagia, 28(1), 69–76. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00455-012-9414-0 cullinan, k. (2006, 29 january). health services in south africa: a basic introduction. health-e news, retrieved from http://www.health-e.org.za/2006/01/29/health-services-in-south-africa-a-basic-introduction/ emanuel, e.j., wendler, d., killen, j., & grady, c. (2004). what makes clinical research in developing countries ethical? the benchmarks of ethical research. journal of infectious diseases, 189(5), 930–937. http://dx.doi.org/10.1086/381709 falsetti, p., acciai, c., palilla, r., bosi, m., carpinteri, f., zingarelli, a. et al. (2009). oropharyngeal dysphagia after stroke: incidence, diagnosis, and clinical predictors in patients admitted to a neurorehabilitation unit. journal of stroke and cerebrovascular diseases, 18, 329–335. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jstrokecerebrovasdis.2009.01.009 frick, p.j., barry, c.t., & kamphaus, r.w. (2009). clinical assessment of child and adolescent personality and behavior. (3rd edn.). new york, ny: springer. geyer, j.d., gomez, c.r., sheppard, a.r., & akhtar, n. (2009). complications of acute stroke. in j.d. geyer & c.r. gomez (eds.), stroke (pp. 250–254). philadelphia, pa: wolters kluwer. groher, m.e. (1997). dysphagia: diagnosis and management. boston, ma: butterworth-heinemann medical. haynes, s.n., smith, g.t., & hunsley, j.d. (2011). scientific foundation of clinical assessment. new york, ny: taylor & francis group. health professions council of south africa. (1988). scope of practice of profession of speech-language pathology. retrieved december 2013, from http://www.hpcsa.co.za/uploads/editor/userfiles/downloads/speech/scope_of_profession_slh_1.pdf heart and stroke foundation of ontario. (2006). management of dysphagia in acute stroke: an educational, annual for the dysphagia screening professional. ontario, canada: heart and stroke foundation of ontario. heckert, k.d., komaroff, e., adler, u., & barrett, a.m. (2009). post-acute re-evaluation may prevent dysphagia-associated morbidity. stroke, 40, 1381–1385. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/strokeaha.108.533489 hinchey, j.a., shephard, t., furie, k., smith, d., wang, d., & tonn, s. (2005). formal dysphagia screening protocols prevent pneumonia. stroke, 36, 1972–1976. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/01.str.0000177529.86868.8d hinds, n.p., & wiles, c.m. (1998). assessment of swallowing and referral to speech and language therapists in acute stroke. quarterly journal of medicine, 91, 829–835. http://dx.doi.org/10.1093/qjmed/91.12.829 langmore, s. (2001). endoscopic evaluation and treatment of swallowing disorders. new york, ny: thieme publishing. logemann, j.a., veis, s., & colangelo, l. (1999). a screening procedure for oropharyngeal dysphagia. dysphagia, 14, 44–51. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/pl00009583 mamdani, m. (2011, may 27). africa’s post-colonial scourge. mail & guardian, retrieved from http://mg.co.za/article/2011-05-27-africas-postcolonial-scourge marik, p.e., & kaplan, d. (2003). aspiration pneumonia and dysphagia in the elderly. chest journal, 124, 328–336. http://dx.doi.org/10.1378/chest.124.1.328 martens, l., cameron, t., & simonsen, m. (1990). effects of a multidisciplinary management program on neurologically impaired patients with dysphagia. dysphagia, 5(3), 147–151. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf02412638 martino, r., pron, g., & diamant, n.e. (2000). screening for oropharyngeal dysphagia in stroke: insufficient evidence guidelines. stroke, 5, 19–30. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s004559910006 martino, r., silver, f., teasell, r., bayley, m., nicholson, g., streiner, d.l. et al. (2009). the toronto bedside swallowing screening test (torr-bsst). stroke, 40, 555–561. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/strokeaha.107.510370 mcburney, d., & white, t.l. (2010). research methods. (8th edn.). belmont, ca: wadsworth cengage learning. murray, j. (2000). manual of dysphagia assessment in adults. san diego, ca: singular publishing group. perlman, a.l., & schulze-delrieu, k.s. (1998). deglutition and its disorders: anatomy, physiology, clinical diagnosis, and management. san diego, ca: singular publishing. perry, l. (2000). screening swallowing function of patients with acute stroke. part one: identification, implementation and initial evaluation of a screening tool for use by nurses. journal of clinical nursing, 10, 463–473. http://dx.doi.org/10.1046/j.1365-2702.2001.00501.x perry, l., & love, c.p. (2001). screening for dysphagia and aspiration in acute stroke: a systematic review. dysphagia, 16, 7–18. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/pl00021290 rust, j., & golombok, s. (1999). modern psychometrics: the science of psychological assessment. (2nd edn.). new york, ny: routledge. schiavetti, n., & metz, d.e. (2002). evaluating research in communicative disorders. (5th edn.). boston, ma: allyn & bacon. schrock, s.a., & coscarelli, w.w.c. (2007). criterion-referenced test development: technical and legal guidelines for corporate training. san francisco, ca: pfeiffer. seedat, j. (2013). knowledge translation in dysphagia – a south african study. doctoral dissertation, department of speech and hearing therapy, university of the witwatersrand, south africa. shipley, k.g., & mcafee, j.g. (2004). assessment in speech-language pathology: a resource manual. (3rd edn.). new york, ny: delmar learning. smith-hammond, c.a., goldstein, l.b., zajac, d.j., gray, l., davenport, p.w., & bolser, d.c. (2001). assessment of aspiration risk in stroke patients with quantification of voluntary cough. neurology, 56, 502–506. http://dx.doi.org/10.1212/wnl.56.4.502 south african speech-language and hearing association (saslha). (2009). guidelines for speech-language pathology and/or audiology service provision in the public sector. document compiled by a. kritzinger, university of pretoria, south africa. tanner, d.c. (2007). medical-legal and forensic aspects of communication disorders, voice prints, and speaker profiling. tucson, az: lawyers & judges publishing company. trapl, m., enderle, p., nowotny, m., teuschl, y., matz, k., dachenhausen, a. et al. (2007). dysphagia bedside screening for acute-stroke patients: the gugging swallowing screen. stroke, 38, 2948–2952. http://dx.doi.org/10.1161/strokeaha.107.483933 wood, j.m. (2007). understanding and computing cohen’s kappa: a tutorial. retrieved december 2013, from http://wpe.info/papers_table.html appendix 1 the south african dysphagia screening tool id e n tity c o n stru c tio n f o llo w in g t ra u m a tic b rain injury: a c a se s tu d y 17 identity construction following traumatic brain injury: a case study a yesha s abat*, legini m oodley#, h a rsh a k a th a r d m' ^ d ep artm en t o f s peech t h erap y & a u d io lo g y , u n iv ersity o f k w az u lu n atal, s o u th a frica ^d epartm ent o f s peech t h erap y & a u d io lo g y , u n iv ersity o f k w az u lu n atal, south a frica >t' d iv isio n o f c o m m u n ic atio n s cien ces and d isorders, u n iv ersity o f c ap e t ow n, s o u th a frica a b s t r a c t this co n stru ctio n o f s e l f id en tity p r e a n d p o st-t ra u m a tic b ra in in ju ry (tbi) in a sin g le case s tu d y is described. a life h isto ry resea rch m e th o d o lo g y w as e m p lo y ed to ex p lo re the ex p erien c e o f a s u rv iv o r o f tbi, u sin g a sin g le case s tu d y design.. the p a rtic ip a n t w as a 31 y e a r o ld w hite s o u th a fric a n m a le who su sta in e d t b i w h ile on d u ty in the arm y. m u ltip le in terview s w ere c o n d u c te d w ith the p a r tic ip a n t to a llo w in -d ep th exp lo ra tio n o f his s e l f id en tity fo r m a tio n p r e a n d p o st-t b i. d a ta a n a lysis en ta ile d tra n scrib in g the interview s, c ra ftin g a resea rch s to r y (n a rra tive an a lysis) a n d an a n a lysis o f th e narrative. the re su lts illu m in a ted the em e rg en c e a n d d ev elo p m e n t o f a re sis­ tance id e n tity as a p r o d u c t o f ea rly p r e -t b i experience, the loss o f s e lffo llo w in g t b i as w ell as the em erg en ce o f a p o s itiv e self-identity. the e m b e d d ed issu es o f co m m u n ic a tio n a n d se lf-id en tity are explained. the p a r tic ip a n t’s n a rra tive esp o u sed a h o p efu l optim ism , stro n g ly ch a llen g in g the d o m in a n t d isa b ility discourse. the sp e cific stj-engths a n d lim itations, a n d p o te n tia l va lu e o f u sin g life h isto ries as b oth a m e th o d o lo g ic a l a n d clin ic a l to o l w hen w o rkin g w ith t b i su rv ivo rs is described. im p lica tio n s f o r resea rch a n d clin ic a l p ra c tic e in the f i e l d o f sp eech a n d l a n g u a g e t herapy (slt) is also p ro vid ed . key w ords: t rau m atic brain injury, n arrativ e life h isto ry m eth o d o lo g y , self-id en tity , life ex p erien ce, resistan c e identity, loss o f self, co m ­ m u n ic atio n im pairm ent. in t r o d u c t io n t rau m atic b rain in ju ry (tb i) exerts a p ro fo u n d im p act on an in d iv id u a l’s life. t h e effects are p erv asiv e and d evastating. a co m p lex range o f co m m u n ic atio n d ifficu lties, in clu d in g specific d istu rb an ces su ch as ap h asia and d y sarth ria, are p ro d u ce d (h ilari & b yng, 2001). in ad d itio n , tb i also results in co g n itiv e, p h y s i­ cal, em o tio n al, b eh av io u ral, an d p sy ch o so cial se q u ela e (h artley, 1995). u ltim ately , the p erso n w ho su rv iv es a t b i has to n eg o tiate a series o f life-alterin g exp erien ces. t his p ap e r draw s on th e life ex p erien ce o f a t b i su rv iv o r to ex p lo re the d ev e lo p m e n t o f self id e n tity p reand post-t b i. t h ere is a b u rg eo n in g in terest in ex p lo rin g ch an g es in id e n ­ tity fo llo w in g illness in the d iscip lin es o f p sy ch o lo g y , so cio lo g y and m ed ical an th ro p o lo g y (frank, 1995). w ith in the n arrativ e re­ search trad itio n , the su b jectiv e ex p erien ce o f t b i (n o c h i, 2000; 1998) has b een explored. s p eech -]an g u ag e th e ra p y (s l t ) p ro fe s ­ sio n als have also w itn essed a tran sitio n in research o rien tatio n to w ard s the u se o f q u alita tiv e research m ethods for ex p lo rin g the co n seq u e n ces o f c o m m u n ic atio n d iso rd ers, su ch as ap h asia (pound, parr, l in d sa y & w o olf, 2000; d am ico, o elsch laeg er, & s im m o n s-m ack ie, 1999; and parr, b yng, g ilpin, & ireland, 1997). t h e cu rren t stu d y is p rem ised on the n o tio n th at k n o w led g e o f se lf-id e n tity inform s clin ical p ractice. it is stro n g ly alig n ed w ith the p o sitio n taken by h ag stro m an d w ertsch (2004), w ho assert th at clin ician s need to u n d ersta n d w ho the p erso n is in o rd er to h elp h im /h er w ith co m m u n icatio n . s ervices w o u ld b e m ore re le ­ v an t i f clin ician s had th e k n o w led g e to u n d erstan d th e in d iv id u al in th e co n tex t o f h is/h er life circu m stan ces. s elf-id en tity fo rm a tio n is a co m p lex p ro ce ss sh a p ed b y p erso n al, so cial and tem p o ral factors (sarbin, 2000). t w o su b sy s­ tem s u n d erlie self-id en tity , n am ely p erso n a l id en tity and social identity. p erso n al id en tity refers to th e u n iq u e co n stitu tio n and b io g rap h y o f the ind iv id u al, su ch as p referen c es and p erso n a lity traits, w h ereas social id en tity refers to d escrip tio n s o f self, b ased on m em b ersh ip and id e n tifica tio n w ith a certain so cial group (de la r e y , 1991). id en tity is a th e o ry o f s e lf th a t is d ev elo p ed and su stain ed th ro u g h a p ro cess o f inner co n c u rre n ce ab o u t w h at th e s e lf is like (s ch len k er, 1987). t he cre atio n o f se lf-k n o w le d g e relies on sev eral co m p o ­ n en ts viz. p erso n a l m em o ry o f th e p ast, co n c ep tu alisin g societal roles and in teractio n w ith th e e n v iro n m en t (n eisser, 1988). a tb i su rv iv o r’s m e m o ry o f p re-m o rb id ex p e rien c e shapes h is/h er self identity, w h ich often has u n d erg o n e a critical ch an g e fo llo w in g the h ead injury. f o llo w in g a m ark ed life-alterin g event, n ew m e an ­ ings are assig n ed to th e in d iv id u a l’s so cial in teractio n s, thus re ­ sh ap in g h is/h er id en tity d ev elo p m en t. n e is s e r’s (1988) co n c ep t o f the ex ten d ed s e lf has im p licatio n s for a co n tin u in g life h isto ry in that in d iv id u als relate th e ir p ast s e lf d uring th e co n stru c tio n o f th eir p re se n t and fu tu re n o tio n s o f th e ir self-identity. a s th e m e an ­ in g an d statu s o f a life ev en t ch an g es for an in d iv id u al o v er tim e, so does h is/h er d efin itio n o f th e self, sin c e a p erso n a l sense o f s e lf co n tain s p ast and p rese n t ex p erien ces and ex p ectatio n s for the fu tu re (b ru m fitt, 1998). t hus, as self-id en tity is co n tin u o u sly c o n ­ stru cted an d reco n stru cted , s l t s n eed to be se n sitiv e to this p ro c ­ ess b y u n d ersta n d in g th e in d iv id u al as a d y n am ic so cial being (h ag stro m & w ertsch , 2004). t h e ex p e rien c e o f co m m u n ic atio n im p a irm en t is em b ed d ed w ith in th e p ro ce ss o f se lf-id e n tity fo rm atio n . t he irony lies in the p ro b lem itself, in th a t co m m u n icatio n , w hich is req u ired as a form o f ex p re ssio n o f self-id en tity , is co m p ro m ised (b ru m fitt, 1998). a n in d iv id u al w ho has su stain ed t b i has to n eg o tiate n ew w ays o f co m m u n ic atin g and en g ag in g in v ario u s so cial activ ities, and re ­ n eg o tiate co m p lex ch allen g es w ith red u c ed flu id ity and flex ib ility (s h ad d en & a gan, 2004). t h ey are ju d g e d b y th o se w ith w h o m th ey en g ag e so cially as eith er co m p ete n t o r n o t b y v irtu e o f the su ccess o f th e ir co m m u n ic ativ e in teractio n s. t h eir selfid en tity rests critica lly on how w ell th ey are ab le to n eg o tiate these in te rac­ tions, as th e ir p ro jec ted id en tities are su b je ct to so cial evaluation. c o m m u n ic atio n is cen tral in sh ap in g se lf-id e n tity fo rm a tio n as the in d iv id u al w ith t b i co n ten d s w ith an altered self, as ch aracterised b y n ew w ay s o f th in k in g , u n d ersta n d in g , ex p ressin g an d sense m aking. the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. s3, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 18 ayesha sabat, legini moodley and harsha kathard w hat challenges m ay slts face when w orking with tbi survivors via a self-identity lens? issues o f identity construction post-tb i m ay seem daunting, since identity constructs have typi­ cally not been w ithin the traditional know ledge base o f slts. self-identity is the traditional domain o f psychology in clinical disciplines and the inclusion o f self-identity in the slt discourse creates the challenge o f negotiating boundary crossing across dis­ ciplines. furtherm ore, slts m ay be reluctant to take on m ore than they should due to large caseloads and lim ited healthcare funding (shadden & agan, 2004). finally, identity loss and reconstruction in the aftermath o f tbi is an elusive concept and m ay be difficult to target as a tangible therapeutic goal. however, given the inex­ tricable link betw een com m unication and self-identity it is neces­ sary to intervene w ithin a fram ew ork that places the w hole indi­ vidual at centre stage. the notion that a sense o f s e lf is m aintained in even the most im paired com m unicator (brumfitt, 1998) sup­ ports the need to understand the preserved se lf o f an individual with acquired com m unication impairment therapeutic intervention m ay be enhanced b y understand­ ing the relationship betw een therapeutic outcom e and self understanding. pound (1993) explored the attitudes o f aphasic speakers and their therapists to aphasia and found that slts who were interviewed focused alm ost exclusively on what they deem ed the devastating consequences o f im paired com m unication follow­ ing stroke. on the other hand, people with aphasia reported both the negative and positive aspects o f their altered lives and equated the importance o f com m unication in their lives with other m ajor life issues, such as driving, em ploym ent and the ability to execute activities o f daily living. therefore, i f clinicians are to provide a service that is person-centred, it is critical to acknowledge that it is not sufficient to focus exclusively on the individual’s com m uni­ cation im pairm ent but to gain a deeper understanding o f other per­ tinent life issues that feature robustly in his/her reality. clinicians therefore should attem pt to understand who the person is in order to effectively m anage the consequences o f com m unication diffi­ culties. traditionally, research on the psychosocial issues pertain­ ing to tbi has been am assed within quantitative research fram e­ works. as a consequence o f the m ethodological choices, the un ­ derstanding o f tbi has been relayed from an etic (or outsider) professional perspective. m any o f these studies have utilised quantitative positivist research designs to explore com plex con­ cepts such as the chronic physical, emotional, and social changes post-tbi; quality o f life following tbi; functional perform ance after tbi; and rehabilitation outcom e follow ing tbi (corrigan, bogner, m ysiw, clinchot & fugate, 2001). the process o f know l­ edge production in the field is therefore likely to benefit from an epistem ological shift w hich creates understanding o f the personal experiences o f people who live in the afterm ath o f tbi. the inclusion o f the experiential dom ain m ay assist in cre­ ating knowledge to support the current paradigm shift in the reha­ bilitation field from the medical m odel tow ard the social model o f disability, w hich em phasises the interaction o f disease and disabil­ ity w ithin the larger am bit o f society (jordan & bryan, 2001). w hile the medical m odel has centralised the focus on impairment, the social model requires a deeper understanding o f disability w ithin a social context o f disabling barriers. social barriers must be rem oved as an essential prerequisite for em powering people with disability (pound et al., 2000). the significance o f generating an integrated understanding o f disability has lead to the blurring o f boundaries betw een slt, sociology and disability theory (pound et al., 2000). in light o f the foregoing discussion, a life history research m ethodology was utilized in this study to explore self-identity formation. the personal biography highlights the issues most im­ portant to the individual in his or her context (atkinson, 1998). the life history narrative allow s participants to engage in a proc­ ess o f self-reflection, thus enabling them to explore and construct self-narratives about them selves located within a broader social, political, and historical milieu, thus generating broader contextual m eaning from the individual’s experience (cole & knowles, 2001). life history research allows the opportunity for the indi­ vidual to ably foreground the se lf in relation to im pairm ent over time (kathard, 2003). life history research offers a heightened sensitivity to the com plexity o f tbi by adding a hum an dim ension to the experi­ ence o f tbi and is particularly valuable in understanding the changing dynamics o f individuals who experience fluctuating perform ance as they recover (hartley, 1995). the story o f a tbi survivor purveys not only what the disability m eans to the indi­ vidual along a temporal continuum , but also conveys the rich per­ sonal m eaning that s/he has attached to it. critically, life history m ethodology has the potential to generate know ledge which slts can apply to their practice especially when w orking in a culturally and linguistically diverse context, such as south africa. this study responds to the follow ing question: what is the nature and the p rocess o f self-identity construction in an individ­ u al who has sustained traumatic brain injury? m eth o d o lo g y a im s o f the study 1. to explore the nature and process o f self-identity construction preand post-tbi 2. to consider the im plications o f the findings for research and clinical practice in the field o f slt research design a qualitative, life history research m ethodology was util­ ized (cole & knowles, 2001) to explore the p articipant’s self identity form ation using a single subject case study design. life history research values depth over breadth, thus the aim is not population representativeness (cole & knowles, 2001). single­ subject m ethodologies are w idely accepted and relevant as a le­ gitim ate technique for exploring the depth o f experience o f apha­ sia and dysfluency (kearns, 1999) and therefore also have appli­ cability for tbi. im portantly, single case study designs used in qualitative research can contribute to practice, theory, social is­ sues and action and thus hold value in facilitating a deeper under­ standing o f the com plex process o f self-identity construction post tbi. p articipant selection criteria purposeful sam pling was im plem ented to select an infor­ m ation-rich case for in-depth study. this strategy is useful when the purpose is to gain a deep understanding o f the particular case, in the absence o f any desire to generalise the findings to all cases (m cm illan & schumacher, 2000). the participant was s e l e c t e d on the basis o f his experience and understanding o f tbi in his lifeworld. the participant was required to: • be an adult who w ould have sustained a closed head injury at least two years prior to the interview and needed to have lived with tbi in order to relate preand post-tbi experience die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) identity construction following traumatic brain injury: a case study 19 . have adequate cognitive ability that would enable him /her to provide a narrative account o f his/her experience o f tbi via a series o f in depth interviews (nochi, 1998) . have an inform ation-rich story to share (nochi, 1998) < have a degree o f self-aw areness and insight into the fact that s/he had sustained tbi in order to provide useful inform ation for theory-building (nochi, 1998) • v oluntarily participate in the study because life history m eth­ odology necessitates a personal sharing o f life experience and sustained engagem ent with the research process (cole & knowles, 2001) • be able to share his/her experience in english so that re­ searcher and participant could com m unicate in a com m on language for the purposes o f facilitating an in-depth explora­ tion o f his/her life experience • be able to share his/her story with adequate verbal com pe­ tency or via alternative or augm entative com m unication if considered appropriate m ethod o f data collection m ultiple in-depth interviews were conducted to enable the researcher to obtain m eaningful insights that reflected how the individual made sense o f the event o f tbi in his/her life. the content o f the interviews was shaped by focussing on topics o f interest or significance to the participant (clandinin & con­ nelly, 2000). see a ppendix a for the interview schedule used i during data collection. the interview schedule was developed | using a tkinson’s (1998) guidelines. the interviews were videoand audio recorded, thus ena­ bling the researcher to observe the kinesics and non-verbal com | m unication o f the participant, lending greater depth to the infor­ m ation shared. audio tape-recording o f the interview sessions i allow ed both the researcher and the participant to cross-check the data gathered, thus enabling a means o f validating the data. the interviews were supplem ented by sources o f inform ation from the participant’s personal journal, photograph album s and m edicalrecords. / r esearch procedure the procedure com prised three main phases, namely, a screening phase, a pilot phase, and main study (table 1). all phases were im plem ented at ja rehabilitation institution in i kwazulu natal, south a frica for individuals who have sustained strokes and head injuries. three participants were interviewed during the screening phase. two participants m et the criteria for selection as they had inform ation-rich stories. one participant was chosen for the pilot and the other as the main case study. d uring the pilot phase, the interview schedule, interview proc­ ess, logistics o f data collection and analysis procedures were refined. in the m ain study the participant was interview ed on three occasions. each interview was approxim ately ninety minutes. the interview s were audio and video recorded. the participant narrated his life story to the researcher. it was critical to establish a com fortable rapport with the participant, in light o f the sensi­ tive and intensely personal nature o f the interviews. exposure o f o n eself during the research process involves issues o f respect, trust, fairness, truth telling and justice (cole & knowles, 2001; c landinin & connelly, 2000). a sem i-structured interview ap­ proach was adopted, thus prom oting flexibility during the inter­ view process (atkinson, 1998). the participant was able to move backw ard and forward along a temporal dim ension as part o f the storytelling process (clandinin & connelly, 2000). description o f the participant the participant selected was a 31 year-old white south african male, paul carstens (name changed to protect identity), w ho sustained tbi on n ovem ber 4, 1991 while on duty in the south african army. he was 18 years old at the time when he was involved in an explosion causing him to sustain a head in­ ju ry and killing his friend who was assisting him. paul was in a com a for six m onths follow ing the accident, having sustained severe brain injury. prior to the accident, paul had ju st com pleted secondary school and embarked on m ilitary service. he described him self pre-m orbidly as a relaxed, fun-loving, unconventional individual who possessed good com m unication and interpersonal skills. at the time o f the interviews, which were conducted in september 2003, at least 12 years post injury, paul was receiving psycho­ therapy and speech-language therapy to address cognitive and com m unication difficulties. in terms o f cognitive abilities, paul was experiencing subtle m em ory, attention, concentration and organisational difficulties. he also presented with m oderate dys­ arthria, thus his speech intelligibility was com prom ised. other conditions included arthritis; restricted m obility (he was using crutches); im paired vision in his left eye due to optical nerve damage; and perm anent dental m isalignm ent caused by injury to his mandible. t able 1: outline o f research procedure ph ase 1: sc r een in g phase 2: pilo t study phase 3: m a in study s three participants were interviewed. since they fulfilled the selection criteria em­ ployed, two were randomly selected for participation in the study. from these two participants, one was chosen for the pilot study and one for the main study. 1 s the participant was interviewed over one one hour and one two-hour sessions. the interviews were audio_ and video recorded. the participant narrated his life story to the researcher. the following aspects were pilot tested: interview schedule, interview process, logistics and data analysis procedures. the participant was interviewed over three one-and-a-half -hour sessions. the interviews were audioand video recorded. the participant narrated his life story to the researcher. the data collected were qualitatively analysed. the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 20 ayesha sabat, legini moodley and harsha kathard ethical considerations qualitative researchers can be considered as guests in the private spaces o f the participants’ world (stake, 1994). there­ fore, it was im perative to adhere to a strict code o f ethics. ethical clearance was obtained from the u niversity o f k w azulu natal ethics committee prior to the study. inform ed consent was ob­ tained from the participant and ethical issues o f anonymity, con­ fidentiality, w ithdraw al and the role o f researcher and participant were highlighted. the participant was treated with fairness by prom oting open discussion and negotiation. he was also in­ form ed o f the intended (video and audio) recording o f the inter­ view sessions. w ritten inform ed consent to record the interviews was obtained from the participant at the outset. in order to ensure an authentic representation o f the participant’s story, he was pro­ vided with the storied (recorded) representation o f his narrative to enable him to validate the information shared (stake, 1994). r igour and trustworthiness the researcher’s reflexive position during the research process is valued as a critical technique for establishing trustw or­ thiness (cole & knowles, 2001). in order to achieve this, num er­ ous strategies o f trustw orthiness were employed. prolonged and persisten t fie ld w o rk involved the use o f m ultiple in-depth inter­ view s and ongoing contact with the participant over the course o f the process spanning two years. the interviews were conducted in the p a rtic ip a n t’s language to ensure that the situation was con­ ducive to storytelling. verbatim accounts o f conversations in the form o f direct quotes were representative o f the participant’s m eaning o f his experience. low inference descriptors refer to literal descriptions used and understood by the participant, as opposed to the abstract language used by the researcher. these add more depth to the data (m cm illan & schumacher, 2000). therefore, in portraying p au l’s account o f his life experience, accurate descriptions from the interviews were used to enhance trustworthiness o f the data. furtherm ore, while in-depth interviews form ed the core data collection technique, triangulation or m ulti-m ethod strate­ gies o f data collection were also em ployed to increase validity (medical records, journals and photo albums). m ultim ethod strategies increase the credibility o f findings by yielding different insights around the central topic (m cm illan & schumacher, 2000). m em ber checking was also employed. this entailed re­ turning the recorded version o f his story to the participant for validation and verification. m em ber checking is an important aspect o f life history m ethodology, as it indicates respect for the individual and enhances the truth-value or fidelity o f the narrative (frank, 1997; a tkinson, 1998). m ethodological challenges the participant was able to narrate his story with consider­ able ease, despite the presence o f dysarthria and subtle cognitive difficulties. organizational and m em ory problem s were m ildly evident during the narration. for example, at certain points in the narration, he reached a blank and was unable to continue with a certain thread o f thought and had to be prom pted. a t other times his narrative tended to be tangential and he thus needed to be re­ directed to the focus o f the question that was asked. fortunately, m em ory blanks were rare as reliance on good m em ory is a criti­ cal aspect o f identity construction (shadden & agan, 2004; neis ser, 1988). interviews were audio_ and video recorded to facilitate the transcription process. the participant and researcher negoti­ ated methods o f clarification, w hich included requesting repeti­ tion and/or elaboration o f a spoken word(s) if they were unclear to the researcher and speaking at an increased vocal intensity to increase the clarity o f his speech. the option o f com m unicating graphically or via the w ritten m ode in instances o f com m unica­ tion breakdown was also available. however, the participant did not find it necessary to use these alternatives. in life history re­ search, particularly when w orking with people experiencing com ­ m unication disorders, it is critical that the researcher is a skilled listener to allow the personal story to unfold meaningfully. d ata analysis the data analysis w as conducted at three levels. the first level o f analysis entailed generating transcripts from the inter­ views conducted. this was followed by narrative analysis (polkinghom e, 1995), w hich produced the research story through m eaningful interaction w ith the interview data (transcripts). the third analytical level involved an analysis o f the narrative (polkinghom e, 1995) in w hich the research story was examined for com m on themes and concepts in relation to the critical re­ search question. d uring this process the researcher steered clear o f im posing prior theoretically derived concepts on the data. instead, careful inspection o f the narrative facilitated the genera­ tion and developm ent o f new concepts from the research data via an inductive analysis. the em erging them es were then discussed in relation to available literature. resu lts and d isc u ssio n for the purposes o f this paper, the results are presented as ex­ cerpts from the research story, as well as verbatim excerpts from the transcripts. the results and discussion are integrated. the em erging themes listed below describe the evolution o f the par­ ticipant’s self-identity: 1. d eveloping a resistance identity as a product o f early pre-tbi experience 2. m oving from loss o f se lf to reconstruction o f se lf post-tbi 3. c onstructing a positive self-identity post-tb i i d eveloping a resistance identity as a product o f early pre-tbi experience 1 i in attempting to understand the process o f identity form a­ tion and the shifts in self-identity, it was critical to explore the role that early childhood experiences have played in shaping p a u l’s self-identity. his early life experiences reflected a child­ hood characterised by disappointm ent, sadness, confusion and a sense o f abandonment. paul experienced resentm ent and deep pain at being separated from his parents at an early age when he was sent to boarding school: ‘we were sent up to eshowe. i begged and p le a d e d f o r her (my mother) to take m e back. i think i was causing her more pa in than anything else. she drove all the w ay to eshowe with us in the car, w hich made her happy. she drove back alone a n d she cried a ll the w ay hom e ... g o in g to school, leaving home ... that had to have been the saddest experi­ ence o f my childhood. ’ p a u l’s feelings o f loneliness and sadness were further intensified die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) identity construction following traumatic brain injury: a case study 21 by the reality that his fam ily unit had been split by his p aren t’s divorce when he was four years old. this was clearly a traumatic experience for him, as reflected in the following excerpt: lm y p a ren ts were divorced when i was f o u r ye a rs old. i was too yo u n g to understand what was happening. i d id n 't even know it was happening. b u t soon enough, the reality o f it was etched into our lives. forever. we w orked around it though. m y p arents made sure o f that. ’ paul could not seek solace in his relationship with his older brother, as they were not close. a s a result, he needed to adopt a strategy that would enable him to survive at boarding school away from all that was fam iliar and safe. he depended on him ­ self. paul developed a firm sense o f individuality that became central to his quest for survival at such a tender age, where paren­ tal love and support is so critical to o n e ’s feelings o f safety, secu­ rity and acceptance: six ye a r-o ld boy wants his mommy. a lone in a strange place. b egged a n d pleaded, p lease take me back. i hated being aw ay fr o m mom. i f e l t hurt, rejected, abandoned. i cried. i waited. they d id n ’t com e back f o r me. this w as it. i had to be strong, smart, independent. f ig h t m y own battles. b e my own person. chris, m y brother, a n d i w e r e n ’t close. i could n ’t depend on him. we fo u g h t a ll the time. i built a p ro tec­ tive w all around me. i needed to f e e l safe, in control. i needed to be me. ’ his experience at school was challenging and m ingled with a sense o f longing for his parents. paul also had a dire need to prove his worth to a father who underm ined his ability: ‘l ife a t school w as f u l l o f challenges, especially when mum a n d d a d were so f a r a w a y...m y d a d a n d i ju s t never clicked... it was a strange relationship that we shared. i think my dad j u s t dem anded too much fro m me. i couldn ’t deliver and he used to g ive m e these lectures about not being go o d enough a t a n yth in g .' p au l’s initial reaction o f w ithdraw ing into a silent safe space within h im self to help him deal with the feelings o f abandonm ent he was experiencing, led to him developing a firm sense o f self identity^ d efining o n e’s self-identity is critical to determ ining how one acts in a given environm ent and relates to others (schlenker, 1987). paul was determ ined to be different; to affirm his individuality: | 7 w as a rebel, especially in high school. the anti-trendy, anti-groupie. chris, on the other hand was very much a groupie. i d id m y own thing} m ade my own rules. a n d broke them! that d id n ’t win me too m any frie n d s b ut i had two really g o o d fr ie n d s who understood me. i f w e d id anything we w ould do it three different ways. each to his own. i d id n 't care f o r convention. i still d o n ’t. i broke all the rules! ’ p a u l’s need to adopt a strong persona and not to succum b to peer pressure and group convention was his w ay o f negotiating this challenge. it was never im portant to him to fit in. his outspoken defiance o f social convention is reflected in his discourse and ultim ately linked to who he was. p au l’s strong sense o f inde­ pendence was further reflected in his assertion that he did not have m any heroes growing up. he cited h im self as his source o f inspiration, indicating a certain lack o f w onder and perhaps even trust in the adults in his lifeworld. paul has attributed the person he has becom e to his own effort: ‘t h a t’s w here i developed a ll m y independence fr o m (boarding school) because i h a d to sta n d on m y own two fe e t ...i was never brought up by m y p a ren ts...w h a t i am now is basically self-created...1 was never around m y parents that m u ch ...m y inspiration to do anything had to come fr o m me. ’ p au l’s experience as w hite, english and m iddle-class unfolded within a broader socio-political context. h is experiences unrav­ elled in south a frica during a turbulent social and political p e ­ riod (1980 to 1991). d uring this tim e m ilitary service was still com pulsory for young w hite males under the previous apartheid regime. the im position o f m ilitary service was not always m et with enthusiasm especially since at the critical age o f 18 years, one is usually looking forw ard to travelling and furthering o n e ’s education. in this particular case, paul indicated a strong dislike for the army: 7 hated the army like hell! ’ ironically, his life-altering event occurred in this very context (the army). his sentim ents could be understood within the con­ text o f his tendency to challenge authority and live life by his rules. p aul’s narrative thus spoke resolutely o f his need to locate h im self in a w orld that he often encountered as unfriendly and cold. w hat paul viewed as being different from the rest was later to becom e his w ay o f adapting to lifestyle changes as a person living with an acquired disability. his sense o f self-identity was thus valued as a means o f claim ing his place in a hostile world. it was w ithin this protective space that he retreated in order to feel safeguarded against sources o f pain and disappointm ent. clearly, initial experiences had created a resistance identity; that is, nega­ tive experiences produced a sense o f resistance to convention and anything that underm ined his self-worth. this resistance was ultim ately em bedded pre-m orbidly in p aul’s sense o f identity . this was his w ay o f surviving. a lthough paul retreated into a protective space, he devel­ oped skills that helped him survive in the face o f adversity. h is encounter o f life as a young person fashioned a resilience that was going to prepare him for a greater challenge later in life. in a sense, p au l’s resistance identity harboured strength o f spirit that led to the critical shift in the w ay he was to view his w orld and his place in it: with further intent to resist the restraints and inher­ ent expectations im posed by society on one who has a disability. p a u l’s process o f self-identity form ation is thus best understood w ithin the context o f his early life experiences. the emerging transition o f his self-identity was “norm al” and expected, how ­ ever it m ay not have taken the trajectory it did, had his life cir­ cum stances been different. m oving from loss o f self to reconstruction o f self following tbi p au l’s initial experience follow ing tbi was characterised by a sense o f loss a loss o f m any functions, including his speech: ‘all m y m ovements in the mouth, like m oving my tongue fro m side to side were very slow there was absolutely nothing com ing o u t ’ he experienced the frustration at not being able to project his voice: the stu p id nurses, they used to run a cold bath a n d p u t m e in. i c o u ld n ’t talk. i c o u ld n ’t say anything. i used to try and hit th e m !’ paul endured a transition at this stage from having a strong and the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 22 ayesha sabat, legini moodley and harsha kathard boisterous voice to support his personality and define his sense o f self, to momentarily losing that voice. a t a broader level, p a u l’s sense o f self experienced a shift which was defined by loss fol­ lowing the tbi. o ther losses included a certain degree o f loss o f mobility, partial loss o f independence and econom ic em pow er­ ment, loss o f form er friendships as friends were uncom fortable about p aul’s acquired disability and loss o f a certain public image that he always tried to project. sense o f self is dram atically affected b y tbi and is experi­ enced by survivors o f tbi as a complex, m ulti-layered phenom e­ non (nochi, 1998). individuals with tbi often experience conflict in trying to reconcile the disparity created by the self-image they m aintain and society’s perception o f who they really are. the dom inant public narrative has the tendency to impose negative labels on people with tbi. this creates conflict w ithin the indi­ vidual, leading to a sense o f loss o f self. n ochi (1998) has identi­ fied this phenom enon as loss o f s e lf as perceived by others. l a­ bels generally im ply negative images o f disability and prom ote a deficit-based, pathologically oriented view o f people with dis­ abilities, thus discounting their individuality and casting them into pre-existing categories. this perspective o f people with dis­ abilities is espoused by the medical model, w hich perpetuates the notion that illness is a form o f social deviancy. p a u l’s resistance identity then gradually resurfaced as his recovery progressed and he began to challenge the public narrative. he regained his speech, his voice, which was a critical step in the transition. he spoke strongly o f his need to be acknow ledged as an individual with unique traits that preclude any form o f categorisation: 7 am priceless. i am unique. e verything about me marks who i am. m y thumbprints. m y voice. i am an individual. a n d no one can take that away fr o m m e.’ [em phasis mine] paul thus once again asserted his identity forem ost as an individ­ ual who has never blindly followed convention. the fact that he was physically challenged did not feature resolutely in his self­ appraisal. he spoke o f his frustration due to the limitations in­ curred by his injuries, such as not being able to drive or su rf any­ more. interestingly, he did not cite his rem aining com m unication difficulties as a constraint im posed by the head injury. instead, he chose to value his voice and saw it as instrum ental in affirming his identity. p a u l’s com m unication difficulties and the utility o f speech-language therapy were raised by the researcher. however, he did not attribute m uch significance to these aspects, choosing to talk about other critical aspects which concerned him e.g. his lim ited mobility. w hilst his speech m ay have had a dysarthric quality, rendering it unclear at times, w hat m attered to paul was that, ultimately, he was able to com m unicate his m essage. d e­ spite the consequences o f his impairm ent/s he did not view him ­ se lf entirely as defined by a disability. the w ay that society p e r­ ceived him however, was apparent to him and to this end, paul experienced the effects o f this negative perception, w hich con­ flicted with his own understanding o f h im self and w hat he was capable o f achieving: ‘life is n ’t always sm ooth sailing when yo u have a disability. people treat y o u differently. the trick is learning how to deal with prejudices. i t ’s n ot easy. som etim es i g e t really angry like when i go to the till to p a y and m y fia n c e is with me and they ask her f o r the money. i t is as though i d o n ’t exist! talk to the m an! ... i have a short fu s e ! i do try to be patien t sometimes. b u t i j u s t g e t so angry when p e o p le talk over me! i wish they w ould see me and treat me like they w ould any­ one else. i know that m y body has been injured b ut m y brain is still happening! ’ [emphasis his] paul faced the challenges o f societal prejudice during his daily course o f events. it was perhaps because o f his perceived com­ m unication (dis)ability and m arked physical im pairm ents, such as his unsteady gait w hilst w alking which necessitated the use of assistive devices like crutches, and his injured eye, that the cash­ ier in the above exchange treated him as described. however, paul used his voice once again to speak out against the prejudice. h e used his ability to com m unicate to his advantage. his experi­ ence o f disability and the subsequent restrictions it imposed on his m ovem ent was exacerbated by the discrim ination displayed towards him, em bodied in a form o f social prejudice that exists against people with disabilities: 7 see that i f i t in perfectly into society. b u t society, i feel, has a problem with me. i am a person! i think that people need to give us a chance. they have to learn to accept and adapt to p eo p le with disabilities. society lim its its e lf by lim ­ iting us. ’. if paul accepted the labels and associated expectations that soci­ ety im posed upon him and his disability, then he would have to relinquish the validation o f his self-image as a unique individual (nochi, 1998). his narrative resonated w ith the sentim ent that he was not w illing to accept society’s labels and was clearly deter­ m ined to carve out a positive self-identity, and receive acknowl­ edgem ent for the person that he is; a determ ination historically rooted and linked to his resistance identity. p a u l’s construction o f self-knowledge and ultim ately the reconstruction o f his self via new experiences is a deeply com plex concept. a ttem pting to engage with this com plex process can only serve to deepen an understanding o f the lived experience o f tbi in all its m ultifari­ ous components. constructing a positive self-identity post-tbi in exploring the process o f positive identity construction post tbi, issues o f attitude, hope and illness as m oral re­ awakening were central features. m oral re-aw akening refers to a renew ed sense o f spirituality that is drawn from an experience o f illness or disability (frank, 1997). a ttitu d in a l dim ensions p au l’s story resonated with courage, hope and an ultim ate acceptance o f all that has occurred in his life thus far, with spe­ cific reference to the accident and resulting injuries. he ac­ know ledged, after an ongoing period o f grieving (at', least four years post injury), that life goes on. w ith this acceptance o f the w ay his life unfolded, paul chose to adopt an attitude that would facilitate his transition into a new life with optim ism and strength o f spirit: 7 have two choices: i can either be happy and accept what is happening a n d look fo rw a rd to the future, or be sa d and re­ g re t what is done ’ p au l’s attitude was infused with hope. he chose to use the ex­ perience o f the accident in his favour. instead o f allow ing him self to be paralysed with sadness, resentm ent and regret at the course his life has taken, paul resolved to pursue his^ challenges with courage and hope. rem arkably, the very event that had caused his life to be steered in a direction he did not anticipate, had also enabled him to discover certain truths about himself: 7 used to be very naive and'carefree. b u t the accident has transform ed m y life, the way i see things. i fe e l more grounded. i ’m able to see the lighter side o f things. i used to die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) identity construction following traumatic brain injury: a case study 23 be a very negative guy. ’ evidently, the accident had changed p au l’s life transform ing the way he saw and experienced his world. he had been thrust from complacency to an appreciation o f life with all its com plexity and contradiction. paul felt that the accident had granted him the op­ portunity to reconstruct h im self positively. m oreover, while paul was initially plagued by feelings o f insecurity and loneliness fos­ tered by a less than ideal fam ily dynam ic, he later (post-tbi) experienced greater stability due to im proved personal relation­ ships. p a u l’s reaction to the accident, resultant injuries and the pervasive im pact it had exerted on his life and sense o f self clearly contradicted the conventional notion that traum atic ex­ periences produce an ongoing sense o f loss and devastation (schlenker, 1987). he chose to com m unicate his responses in a positive way, thus constructing a positive social identity, which in turn influenced his actions and responses to certain life events. while the experience o f loss is a real part o f the experience, paul’s story highlights the fact that it does not continue indefi­ nitely, thus contradicting the m edical notion that people live in a continual state o f loss. m oral re-awakening p au l’s self-narrative echoed with hope and optim ism for the future. his plans included sharing his life with his significant other and creating vocational opportunities for himself. he em­ phasised the role o f spirituality in his positive reconstruction: ‘the one and only thing i can rem em ber fr o m m y coma is saying god, p lea se d o n ’t let me go. i was literally begging g od ju s t to let me live, especially f o r my mother. since then ' religion has becom e so im portant to me. i t ’s brought m y life into perspective. i t has made me realise w hat is really impor­ tant to me. i value m y mom a n d m y fia n c e equally. a n d my religion supersedes them. i have also learned to take g o o d care o f myself, to value m y general well-being. a n d fa m ily is the m ost im portant thing to me. ’ in this vein, p a u l’s narrative is closely aligned with f rank’s (1995) quest narrative, w hich encounters suffering boldly, ac­ cepts illness and uses it heroically. q uest narratives involve per­ severance and are oriented tow ard the future. illness is thus not viewed as an interruption that m ust be overcome b ut rather as a challenge that must be m et andj from which the individual can gain som ething meaningful. illness is seen as a critical event that becom es an occasion to turn an inner gaze and to engage in a moral questioning o f who we arel(frank, 1997). p a u l’s narrative reflects the polyphonic nature o f a quest narrative w here m ultiple voices echo complex, interw oven and often contradictory values and stories and represent an affirm a­ tion o f identity (frank, 1995). thus, in p a u l’s life story, living with an acquired disability was n ot view ed entirely as fatalistic but was em braced as part o f his com plex life experience. the sense o f hope that paul fostered represented new meanings that he created o f h im self through the experience o f trauma. k leinm an (1988) contends that the experience o f a critical event need not be experienced as a self-defeating exercise but can be used as a con­ duit to deeper and better things. hope is b o m from the personal and cultural dilemmas that are induced by illness, forcing one to turn to sources o f m eaning that are already present in our lives (kleinman, 1988). a lthough paul was often plagued by despair and repeatedly questioned his fate, there was an overriding sense o f resolution and motivation: ( 7 believe that g od is supreme. h e is in charge o f absolutely everything. a n d i f h e w anted this to happen, there had to be a way. i t h a d to be h is will. i f g od is willing, who am i to question that? h e has helped to improve m y life. things can only g e t better. i believe that g od gives us gifts. i f we d o n 't use them, he takes them away a n d in so doing takes a chunk o f our lives away. m y g ift fro m h im is to help p eo p le by sh a r­ ing m y experiences with them a n d i ’m trying to p u t back into society w hat i ’ve taken out. to search within m y se lf a n d make a difference to o th e rs’ lives. t h a t’s why these m otiva­ tional talks that i give are so im portant to me. sharing m y experiences also helps me com e to term s with w here i am right now. ’ through his spiritual growth, paul was able to reach out to others, sharing his experiences with them and strengthening his self identity. paul saw his ability to do this as a divine gift and in so doing recognised that his own life experiences could be used as valuable lessons not only to others but to h im self as well. h is desire to help others resonates w ith.k leinm an’s (1988) suggestion that illness heightens o n e’s aw areness o f other peo p le’s suffering, thus m aking one m ore inclined to behave in ways that would help to reduce the suffering o f others and allow one to assume respon­ sibility for others. the m otivational talks paul presented, p ro ­ vided a valuable opportunity for his personal growth following the accident. in giving to others, p a u l’s feelings o f self-worth were reaffirm ed. he used his com m unication skills positively and to his benefit, despite the dysarthria. paul was able to view the events o f his life as m eaningful and purposeful, thus em pow­ ering him w ith hope and the ability to perceive and m aintain an im portant role in society. it is alm ost as if, in living a post traum atic life, p aul’s traum a has becom e the source o f his work, which in turn has becom e a source o f community, nurturing po s­ sibilities o f new role-taking and m eaning-m aking (frank, 1997). im plic a tio n s, rec o m m en d a tio n s and lim itatio ns the study holds significant im plications at various levels. the contributions o f life history research are linked to its intellec­ tual and moral purpose and its potential to provide insight into individual lives (cole & knowles, 2001). the present study holds the potential to provide a research m ethodological tool for exploring in greater depth the lives o f the people w ith whom cli­ nicians work. life history research can be utilised to engage at deeper levels w ith individuals who experience tbi and its conse­ quences, as illustrated in p au l’s case. im portantly, the study has also shown that even when p eo ­ p le present with com m unication im pairm ent they have stories to tell and as such should be granted the opportunity to be heard. life history research has the potential to provide a platform upon w hich discourse around individuals with disabilities, who have been m arginalized from the dom inant research dialogue, may be informed. p au l’s story highlighted this notion and illum inated the com plexity o f issues underlying self-identity and com m unication. the use o f life histories as a clinical m ethod can be ex­ plored w ithin slt contexts. w hile com m unication is the focus o f intervention, it is also pivotal to creating self-identity. therefore, when applied to the understanding o f an individual who has sur­ vived a tbi, com m unication is the instrum ent used to generate a life story account o f experience. b y understanding the critical dual role o f com m unication as both an intervention need, as well as in constructing self-identity, clinical interventions can becom e m ore client-centred. the know ledge produced in this study can thus encourage clinicians to understand their clients' com m unica­ the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 24 ayesha sabat, legini moodley and harsha kathard tion needs as they relate to the individual’s sense o f se lf in his/her life context. w hen com m unication is view ed in this way, it m ay assist the clinician in thinking through intervention goals for the individual and to understand the types o f intervention that are necessary. clinicians who have read p au l’s story at a general hospital in johannesburg, south a frica and sandwell, england have indi­ cated that life history has potential as a clinical tool. w hilst clini­ cians often use a case history, this tool traces the history o f the disorder while the life history interview offers potential to under­ stand how individuals live with disorders. b y developing client centred therapy goals, the clinician is able to generate relevant functional com m unication intervention. they also reported that using life histories in therapy w ould highlight the importance o f other life issues (em ploym ent, housing, relationships, physical m obility, inability to drive) w hich are im portant to consider when setting intervention goals and strategies. a lthough com m unica­ tion was an im portant issue, it rem ained em bedded within an identity frame. however, time constraints were cited as a con­ cern and lim itation to the clinical im plem entation o f life histories. furtherm ore, the slts reported that reading this particular narrative challenged their perceptions o f people with disabilities as they acknow ledged that positive experiences could emerge from an event such as tbi. this notion resonates with p ound’s (1993) assertion that it is critical for slts to develop a height­ ened awareness and deeper understanding o f the m ulti-faceted nature o f individuals’ responses to disability, in order to appreci­ ate a different perspective and to cultivate positive attitudes amongst professionals. she m aintained that if therapists adopt negative attitudes to disability by focusing on impairm ent, it is likely to lead to negative stereotyping o f people w ith disabilities. this attitude hinders the recognition o f the potential o f people w ith disabilities to take control over their changing lives. clini­ cians are encouraged to critically evaluate their own beliefs and attitudes to ensure that their practice removes, rather than creates, disabling barriers. w hen applied to p a u l’s case, it w ould have been easy to interpret p a u l’s experiences in general, and his com ­ m unication difficulties in particular, as negative events. his nar­ rative, however, proved otherwise. in the field o f tbi particularly, slts m ust be cognisant o f w hat role they can play in strengthening the positive self-identity trajectory. the com m unication intervention program therefore depends on the clinician’s ability to consider issues o f the client’s potential and his/her understanding o f disability. in p a u l’s story for example, his optim istic appraisal o f experience could well be overlooked in a therapy program that is not sensitive to the com ­ plex processes underlying the construction o f his positive self identity post tbi. it w ould be imperative to engage w ith p a u l’s affirm ing attitude and inner self if relevant therapy goals and in­ tervention plans are to be produced. the intim ate link between his com m unication and self-identity w ould be the key to fostering optim al intervention. life history research also engages with the spiritual and m oral dimensions o f the individual. as discussed, p au l’s story resonated w ith a sense o f moral awakening. his experience o f tbi has ignited a deeper life m eaning and spiritual awakening that forms the basis o f how he lives, com m unicates and interacts with people. through understanding these deeper dimensions o f the individual, clinicians m ay be able to sharpen their insight into the m ultifaceted nature o f hum an experience. w hile the body/ im pairm ent aspects o f interventions have received prim ary atten­ tion in the speech-language pathology literature, issues o f spiri­ tuality w ould appear to have received marginal coverage (jordan & bryan, 2001). h owever, p au l’s story implies that clinicians should understand and interact with issues o f spirituality where they are apparent i f they are to engage with personally meaningful interventions. the use o f narratives in aphasia therapy is endorsed by pound et al. (2000) who encourage slts to sharpen their listening skills and hone in on their ability to facilitate storytelling, as the value o f narrative m edicine can be used as a pow erful tool for fuelling therapeutic interventions and m easuring outcom es. the very act o f telling o n e’s story has potential to produce a therapeu­ tic and cathartic effect for the storyteller. em pirical research that has been conducted on the use o f narrative constructions with diverse populations experiencing chronic conditions has indicated that narratives o f emotional experiences have produced positive changes in these individuals (nochi, 2000; parr et al., 1997). practically, for the individuals whose lives were irrevoca­ b ly affected by the experience o f tbi, this study could be used to inform and educate the public about the issues raised. societal notions o f disability need to be challenged to facilitate the suc­ cessful integration o f people with disabilities. again, intervention should encompass the gam ut o f the personal experience o f dis­ ability, extending beyond a focus on the personal to the social creation o f disability. the study has social and political implica­ tions in that the issues o f disability that have been raised have the potential to influence the w ay society perceives people with dis­ abilities. instead o f view ing “pathology” and “norm al” as dispa­ rate entities, they can be seen and appreciated as varieties along a continuum o f m odes o f being-in-the-w orld (papadim itriou, 2001, p. 10), thus encouraging clinicians as well as the public to appreci­ ate disability as a continuum o f hum an diversity. a t a theoretical level, the issues raised in the study are congruent with the m ove tow ard em bracing the social model of disability and procuring subjective perspectives on the experience o f disability (hilari & byng, 2001; frank 1997; n ochi, 2000; n o­ chi, 1998; parr et al., 1997). the study contributes to a growing body o f research on the personal m eaning ascribed to certain life experiences, such as acquiring a disability. furtherm ore, the study has highlighted issues o f com m unication that are embedded within a self-identity development. the strengths o f the study include the data collection m ethod, nam ely a series o f multiple, in-depth interviews, thus yielding value laden, inform ation-rich data. furtherm ore, by virtue o f the life history m ethodology im plem ented, an emic (insider) perspective is provided, as the narrator was able to con­ struct his self-identity and life via the narrative process. a pilot study was conducted in order to evaluate the efficacy o f the inter­ view schedule (data collection tool), interview process, data analysis, and the overall logistics o f the interview procedure, thus enhancing the trustw orthiness o f the main study. a single case study design facilitated an in-depth exploration and understanding o f what it means to be a survivor o f tbi. m oreover, the research story was analysed qualitatively, facilitating insight and a deeper understanding o f the experience o f tbi w ithin the broader land­ scape o f the participant’s lifeworld. as described under participant selection criteria, the par­ ticipant was required to have sufficient cognitive and com m unica­ tive ability to be included in the study. these were not stipulated as exclusionary criteria, but rather to facilitate the process and provide relevant know ledge about the particular case. since this is still a developing m ethodology, future research could look to including people with severe cognitive and com m unication diffi­ culties to expand the know ledge base. this w ould require refin­ ing the current m ethodology considerably. for example, encour­ die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) identity construction following traumatic brain injury: a case study 25 aging the narrator to em ploy a lternative and a ugm entative communication (aac) devices, drawing, symbols, and ges­ tures to convey a m eaningful story. the stance o f the re­ searcher in this instance m ay also change. instead o f using open ended questions, s/he m ay need to use forced alternative questioning in order to procure inform ation from the narrator. a limitation worth noting is that im posed by the con­ straints o f language and text. however, even within linguistic and textual restrictions, it is possible to provide a m eaningful representation o f one’s life within the text (clandinin & c on­ nelly, 2000). m em ory also im poses restrictions on the narra­ tive, as events are selectively revealed, thus despite prolonged engagement with the participant, his/her revelations will always be constrained. this is, however, the nature o f retelling experi­ ence but should be considered particularly when interviewing participants who have m em ory problem s. conclusion this study explored the process o f self-identity construc­ tion pre and post tbi, using a life history m ethodology. the results highlighted the interaction o f com plex variables underly­ ing the process o f self-identity form ation in a survivor o f tbi. the self is seen as central to action. the role o f com m unication in negotiating and representing self-identity is illuminated. the participant’s narrative highlighted his life experience in the p e­ riod preceding and following a head injury. the story revealed how critical early life experiences, fashioned through interac­ tion with significant others (family, peers), shaped self-identity and the ability to negotiate significant life changes incurred by the head injury. the evolution o f a positive self-identity is traced. p au l’s self-identity was constructed via his dialogue. his com m unication was entw ined with all aspects o f his life and served to present a particular social persona. paul used his communication strengths positively in constructing and affirm ­ ing his altered post injury self-identity. illness as an experience o f moral awakening is highlighted, as p au l’s deep spirituality enabled him to nurture hope for the future, to look forward to living and sharing his life with his significant other. in this re­ gard, paul has becom e the ultim ate architect o f his own social reality. a c k n o w ledg em ents this article is based on a dissertation submitted to the u niver­ sity o f k w azulu n atal b y the first author, under supervision o f the second and third authors, m s ilegini m oodley and prof. har sha kathard, in partial fulfilm ent o f the requirem ents for the degree m asters in c om m unication pathology (speechreferences atkinson, r. (1998). the life story interview. newbury park, califor­ nia: sage publications, inc. brumfitt, s. (1998). the measurement of psychological well being in the person with aphasia. international journal o f communica­ tion disorders, 33, 116-120. clandinin, d.j., & connelly, f.m. (2000). narrative inquiry: experi­ ence and story in qualitative research. san francisco: jossey bass publishers. cole, a.l., & knowles, j.g. (eds.). i (2001). lives in context: the art o f life history research. england: alta mira press. corrigan, j.d., bogner, j.a., mysiw, w.j., clinchot, d., & fugate, l. (2001). life satisfaction after traumatic brain injury. journal o f head trauma rehabilitation, 16(6), 543-555. damico, j.s., oelschlaeger, m., & simmons-mackie, n. (1999). qualita­ tive methods in aphasia research: conversation analysis. aphasiology, 13(9-11), 661-679. de la rey, c. (1991). intergroup relations: theories and positions. in d. foster & j. louw-potgieter. (eds.). social psychology in south af­ rica. johannesburg: lexicon publishers frank, a.w. (1997). illness as moral occasion: restoring agency to ill people. health: an interdisciplinary journal fo r the social study o f health intervention and medicine, ,01(02), 131-148. frank, a.w. (1995). the wounded storyteller: body, illness and ethics. chicago: university of chicago press. hagstrom, f., & wertsch, j.v. (2004). grounding social identity for pro­ fessional practice. topics in language disorders, 24(3), 162-173. hartley, l. (1995). cognitive-communicative abilities following brain injury. california: singular publishing group, inc. hilari, k., & byng, s. (2001). measuring quality of life in people with aphasia: the stroke specific quality of life scale. international journal o f communication disorders, 36, 86-91. jordan, l., & bryan, k. (2001). seeing the person? disability theories and speech and language therapy. international journal o f language and communication disorders, 36, 453-458. kathard, h., pillay, m., samuel, m., & reddy, v. (2004). genesis o f self identity as disother: life histories of people who stutter. south afri­ can journal o f communication disorders, 44 (1), 109-117. kathard, h. (2003). life histories o f people who stutter. unpublished doctoral dissertation. durban, kwazulu-natal, south africa: univer­ sity of durban-westville. kearns, k.p. (1999). qualitative research methods in aphasia: a welcome addition. aphasiology, 13(9-11), 649-650. kleinman, a. (1988). the illness narratives: suffering, healing and the human condition. new york: basic books. mcmillan, j.h., & schumacher, s. (2000). research in education: a conceptual introduction. new york: longman, inc. neisser, u. (1988). five kinds o f self-knowledge. philosophical psy­ chology, i, 35-59. nochi, m. (2000). reconstructing self-narratives in coping with trau­ matic brain injury. social science & medicine, 51(2000), 1795-1804. nochi, m. (1998). loss of self in the narratives o f people with traumatic brain injuries: a qualitative analysis. social science & medicine, 46 (7), 869-878. papadimitriou, c. (2001). from disability to difference: conceptual and methodological issues in the study o f physical disability. in s.k. toombs (ed.). the handbook o f phenomenology and medicine. dordrecht: kluwer academic press. parr, s., byng, s., gilpin, s., & ireland, c. (1997). talking about apha­ sia. buckingham: open university press. polkinghome, d.e. (1995). narrative configuration in qualitative analy­ sis. in j.a. hatch & r. wisniewski (eds.). life history and nar­ rative. london: the falmer press. pound, c. (1993). attitudes to disability: power and the therapeutic rela­ tionship. conference paper, british aphasiology society confer­ ence. warwick university. the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 26 ayesha sabat, legini moodley and harsha kathard pound, c., parr, s., lindsay, j., & woolf, c. (2000). beyond aphasia: therapies fo r living with communication disability. bicester, uk: winslow press. sarbin, t.r.a. (2000). worldmaking, self and identity. culture and psychology, 6(2), 253-258 negative life events: clinical and social psychological perspectives new york: plenum press. shadden, b.b., & agan, j.p. (2004). renegotiation o f identity: the social context of aphasia support groups. topics in language disorders 24(3), 174-186 schlenker, b.r. (1987). threats to identity: self-identification and sostake, r.e. (1994). case studies. in n.k. denzin & y.s. lincoln (eds.), cial stress. in c.r. snyder & c.e. ford. (eds.). coping with handbook o f qualitative research. london: sage publications ltd. appendix a interview schedule the fo llo w in g questions were u sed as a gu ide during the interview. the interviews u nfolded in the direction the pa rticip a n t took, with som e guidance being offered by the researcher. ❖ w here / when were you bom , earliest childhood m em ories, what was your fam ily like, describe your parents, siblings, grandparents? ❖ tell me about your linguistic, cultural, social and racial background ❖ tell me about the most significant events in your life ❖ w hat academ ic qualifications do you have? ❖ tell me about your earliest childhood memories ❖ w hat were your experiences like at school? ❖ do you see education as playing an im portant role in o n e’s life? ❖ w ho were your role models? ❖ w hat did you want to becom e on graduating from school? ❖ describe the political climate w hen you were growing up ❖ tell me m ore about the accident ❖ how has having sustained a head injury changed your life? ❖ h ow do you view disability? ❖ w hat are the challenges you face as a person with a disability? ❖ h ow has the accident im pacted on your relationships with fam ily and friends ♦♦♦ d id you find the rehabilitation services rendered to you follow ing the accident effective? ❖ w hat are your hopes for the future? ❖ is there anything we m ay have om itted from your life story? ❖ do you have any com m ents about the interviews and all that has been discussed? ❖ d id you find that talking about your life was a therapeutic experience? ❖ d id it have a cathartic effect on you? die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 51 configurations of self-identity formations of adults who stutter harsha kathard vivienne norman mershen pillay school of health and rehabilitation sciences, university of cape town correspondence to: h kathard (harsha.kathard@uct.ac.za) clinicians require a close understanding of the experience of stuttering in order to offer meaningful and personally relevant interventions for adults who stutter. the individual’s unique experience of stuttering can be revealed through understanding how they construct themselves, i.e. their self-identity. silverman (2001) encouraged growth of researchers’ scientific repertoires and argued that when a person who stuttered sought help, he/she did so as a whole person who lived in complex ways with stuttering. silverman’s plea for such personal knowledge was a result of her observation that research which was reduced to numbers and facts had little practical value for the clinician. noting this methodological and conceptual gap in research, the study (kathard, 2003) upon which this article is based, examined how adults who stutter made sense of themselves, having lived with stuttering since childhood. kathard’s study (2003) was preceded by studies that emphasised the importance of understanding self and stuttering (fransella, 1968, 1972; shearer, 1961; sheehan, 1970). these early theorists offered varied explanations about self, self-construction and stuttering and applied them to interventions. sheehan (1970) explained stuttering by using ‘role-conflict’ theory and considered it as a role-specific behaviour. in his view stutterers have dichotomised understandings of themselves (in role as fluent speaker and role as stutterer). as a consequence of living with stuttering over a prolonged period, they developed understandings of themselves as stutterers rather than as fluent speakers, and this role was resistant to change. while he argued that an acceptance of selfconcept in a stutterer role was important at an initial stage of treatment, it was expected that by the end of an intervention the stutterer would accept himself as a normal speaker by seeing himself in a fluent speaker role. the intervention therefore required a radical reinvention of self to that of a normal speaker. shearer (1961) also described stutterers as having dichotomised and conflicting self-concepts – ‘a horrible stuttering self ’ and a ‘free-speaking normal self ’ (p. 115). the intervention was directed at reconciling these conflicting self-concepts with the intention of achieving a free-speaking normal self. he added that relapse occurred when the stutterer no longer perceived him/herself as a stutterer and therefore did not monitor his/ her speech and the environmental cues which elicit stuttering. fransella (1968) investigated the self-concept of a stutterer as a person using kelly’s personal construct theory (pct). kelly (1955) made three important contributions that have relevance for this study. firstly, he argued that in order to understand the human condition and behaviour, one has to understand it from the vantage point of the person living with that behaviour – the insider view. secondly, when a person makes sense of him/herself he/she does so by generating self-constructs which occur as a network of constructs in relationship with each other. thirdly, he coined the term ‘constructive alternativism’ which elucidated the idea that people have alternatives in how they choose to construct themselves, e.g. fat/thin, happy/sad. the process of successful change is achieved when a person is able to construct an alternative understanding of him/herself as the basis for effecting the necessary behaviour change. fransella (1968) investigated how the person who stutters constructed him/herself as a person using a combination of repertory grid and semantic differential techniques. using this method, she reported that stutterers have two primary understandings of themselves – as an individual and as a speaker. firstly, as an individual he/she is unique and different to all other people, including those who stutter. secondly, as a speaker, he/she holds the negative stereotypical view of themselves as stutterers. the intervention was therefore aimed at helping the stutterer as a speaker to develop a network of self-constructs to revise his/her selfconstruction to that of a fluent speaker and to experiment with fluency. fransella (1972) further tested her hypothesis that stuttering would decrease as the person took on the fluent speaker role. intervention focused on improving the meaningfulness of the fluent speaking role, rather than improving fluency, in adults with developmental stuttering. her hypothesis was confirmed and this strengthened the impetus for self-reconstruction as a basis for achieving improved fluency. evesham and fransella (1985) were also concerned about relapse and hypothesised that stutterers were less likely to relapse if they were able to construct themselves as fluent speakers. they tested their hypothesis with a group of 48 participants who received fluency training in the first week of intervention. participants were then divided into a technique group – intervention focused on prolonged speech techniques – and a pct group who were assisted in reconstructing their fluent speaker role. while all subjects improved their fluency, it was concluded that the pct group had a lower relapse rate (measured by fluency) than the technique group. despite the reported benefit of pct in fluency intervention by evesham and fransella (1985) and support from abstract it is important for clinicians to understand how adults who stutter construct who they are, i.e. their self-identity, in order to offer personally meaningful interventions. early research on stuttering and self-concept provided initial knowledge, but there has been a dearth of further research in this field. this article, the third in a series of papers emanating from a doctoral study, provides new insight into the configurations of self-identity formations of adults who stutter. previous articles from the study described how ‘able’ (positive self-identity) and ‘disother’ (negative self-identity) self-identity formations developed over time. this paper describes the configurations of self-identities, able and disother, evident across participants’ stories. the study used a life-history methodology in which 7 adult participants (5 men and 2 women) shared their experiences of living with stuttering through open-ended interviews. the data from the interviews were analysed at two levels. the first level of analysis resulted in a description of the types of self-identity formations (positive and negative) and processes (personal, social and temporal) shaping the self-identity formations. this result was represented as a research story for each participant. the second level of analysis described the configurations of the self-identities, able and disother, across the research stories. the main findings, illustrated through three research stories, were that the two types of self-identity formations, able and disother, were present in singular and dual configurations. the dual presentation of self-identity formations occurred in co-existing, competing and coalescing configurations at particular time periods, illuminating the complexity of the stuttering experience. the clinical implications are discussed. keywords: life-history methodology, self-identity configurations, stuttering 52 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 self-identity formations of adults who stutter stewart and birdsall (2001), pct did not have prominence in the us literature (dilollo, niemeyer & manning, 2002). while the value of self-reconstruction as a basis for change is recognised in stuttering research, it is also evident that most interventions require the stutterer to take on a fluent speaker role or self-reconstruction as a fluent speaker with intention to improve fluency. although fransella (1968) acknowledged that stutterers had self-constructions of themselves as unique beings, her intervention did not build on these attributes but was directed instead towards self-reconstruction as a fluent speaker. kathard’s (2003) study added to the knowledge base by approaching the study of self-identity as the basis for understanding the personal experience of stuttering. it differed from prior studies on self as it focused on describing the development of self-identity formations as well as the personal, social and temporal processes shaping these selfidentity formations in adults living with developmental stuttering. to achieve this, a purposefully created self-identity theoretical framework coupled with a narrative life history methodology was used. the theoretical framework differentiated between role and selfidentities. while people play many different roles they only become ‘identities’ when people internalise understandings of themselves (mishler, 1999), i.e. an identity of being a stutterer only occurs when the individual attaches such meaning to him/herself. these self-identities are constructed from different subject or ‘i’ positions, e.g. as a child at school, as a child at play, as an adult in a meeting, emphasising that identity formations are constructed from several subjective positions which vary across contexts, relationships and over time (somers, 1994). a person may also construct more than one self-identity of himself/ herself. critical to this paper is the understanding that self-identities are also dialogic, i.e. when an individual has more than one self-identity, these identities occur in varying relationships to each other and may be, for example, in conflictual or co-operative relationships (hermans, 2002), with power dynamics between them influencing their relative dominance. previous studies have taken primarily a psychological approach to self-identity formation. this study combined the ‘bodily’ experience of stuttering, the thoughts, feelings, actions and motivations (psychological), as well as the social and temporal processes that influence identity formation. the social processes highlight how self-identity is shaped by social values and actions evident in daily interactions with family, friends, schools and other social institutions. simply put, if people who are significant in one’s life construct stuttering as a bad thing and broader society holds a similar view, then the person who stutters may internalise this negative influence. it was also acknowledged that lives are dynamic and over time an individual may develop new or revised understandings of themselves. these changing formations suggest that self-identities are always in ‘process’ of developing from past to present and through to the future (mishler, 1999). over time, self-identities could be stable or flexible (valsiner, 2002) as they are shaped by changing social and personal dynamics in everyday circumstances. a qualitative, narrative life-history methodology was best suited to research self-identity formations within the framework described above. qualitative research in general has gained popularity in communication science research because it offers varied research approaches, all of which are analytical by nature and systematic in their execution (cresswell & miller, 2000). such research is valuable in communication disorders because it seeks to understand complex, sociocultural phenomena through exploration, generates rich descriptive data, encourages a focus on individuals in context, and values the participant’s point of view (dilollo & wolter, 2004; tetnowski & damico, 2001). a narrative, story-telling approach was used because stories are considered the best way to understand experience (clandinin & connelly, 1994). in telling their story, individuals combine their past actions and life events into a whole narrative form (polkinghorne, 1996) which illuminates their self-identity. the self-story is ideal because it reveals fuller life configurations, weaving together bodily/ stuttering experiences, emotions, thoughts, contexts, actions, social systems, power and values. participants use words combined as whole, meaningful stories, considered as valid data in studies of self-identity (plummer, 2001). the life-history orientation was applicable because it provided the opportunity to obtain first-hand, retrospective, historical accounts of individuals’ personal experiences (hatch & wisniewski, 1995; plummer, 2001). narrative methodology also allows the participant the freedom to construct him/herself against the dominant discourses influenced by the medical model which traditionally present a view of living with impairment as a personal tragedy. this article is the third in a series of articles emanating from the kathard (2003) study. the first article (kathard, pillay, samuel & reddy, 2004) described the processes shaping the negative self-identity as ‘disother’. the term was coined to explain an individual’s self-identity as different or ‘other’, specifically on the basis of stuttering as a (dis)order. the term (other), appropriated from postcolonial studies (boehmer, 1995), is a relational concept in which the individual constructs him/herself as different (other) in a negative way. this negative self-identity and strategies for self-management were similar to those described in previous studies (fransella, 1968; petrunik & shearing, 1983; shearer, 1961; sheehan, 1970). interrelated social processes (e.g. labelling, norming, judging) combined with personal processes (e.g. discoveries of difference; negative self-judgement, self-colonisation) and temporal burdening resulted in the strengthening and sedimenting of selfidentity as disother. the article also highlighted the importance of factors such as gender, race, culture and stuttering which intersect in self-construction. in the second article in the series kathard (2006) describes the processes underlying the discrete formation of the positive self-identity as able, similar to self as a unique being referred to by fransella (1968). selfidentity as able was based on participants’ positive sense of self as a fuller human being and was not confined to a self-limiting description related to stuttering. participants strengthened and reinforced selfidentity as able by using enabling philosophies for self-development; resourcing themselves to create advantage, broadening their framework for communication and how to enhance it; and developing emotional literacy, personal acceptance of themselves and creating counternarratives. the multiple processes leading to strengthening self-identity as able included improving communication and fluency, but were not limited to it. while previous articles described the processes shaping the discrete self-identity formations able and disother, the aim of this paper is to describe the varying configurations of these self-identity formations evident across the research stories of participants in the study. given the word limitations for this manuscript, excerpts are drawn from three research stories which provide robust illustrations of the findings. method participants participants were recruited through a local stuttering self-help group, hospital-based speech-language pathology units, private practices and general public notices. purposive sampling was used to select those with information-rich stories (plummer, 2001). criteria for inclusion were that they: (i) had stuttered since early childhood; (ii) had informationrich life stories; (iii) were willing to share their stories in english or the language of their choice; and (iv) were able to commit to prolonged engagement with the researcher through a minimum of three in-depth interviews. a fluency assessment was conducted by a qualified speechlanguage therapist at the university clinic to confirm the presence of developmental stuttering, and a brief screening interview ensured that participants had information-rich stories. information-rich stories were those which had details about critical events, were contextualised, and had the necessary personal, temporal and social elements. five men and two women of varied race, age, social and occupational backgrounds vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 53 self-identity formations of adults who stutter were selected to participate in the original study (kathard, 2003). however, excerpts from three of those participants were included in this article to illustrate the results within the word limits of the manuscript. procedure the research process was explained and written consent to participate was obtained before the in-depth individual interviews (rubin & rubin, 1995). during these life-history interviews the researcher created open-ended opportunities for participants to share stories about their experiences with stuttering throughout their lifespan to achieve an in-depth and interconnected understanding (samuel, 2009) of their nuanced life experiences. the interviews were not intended to focus on therapy experiences, unless relevant to the individual’s experience. a significant effort was made to develop a respectful and trusting research relationship (measor & sikes, 1992), to create an environment conducive to truthful personal story-telling. because participants stuttered, the researcher and participants discussed ways to promote easy and respectful communication (kathard, 2009) during the interviews. indepth interviewing was achieved using a series of matrix-like probes to explore personal, social and temporal (kathard et al., 2004) aspects of their story systematically. participants were also encouraged to provide additional documents (e.g. letters, diaries), support material (e.g. video recordings) and artefacts (certificates, photos) to reinforce the authenticity and depth of the story-telling process. two senior researchers evaluated each recorded interview and made suggestions for enhancing the quality of interviews. their suggestions included strategies for enhancing appreciative listening, ways to allow the story to unfold freely, and the strategic use of probes. all participants were interviewed at least three times with each interview lasting approximately 2 hours. the total interview time for each participant ranged from 6 to 10 hours. the researcher engaged with participants over a period of 1 year for the purposes of data collection, review of stories and analysis. all interviews were audio-recorded, transcribed verbatim, checked for accuracy by the researcher and research assistant, and verified by a senior researcher. the study received ethical approval from the university of durban-westville ethic committee. pseudonyms have been used to ensure participants’ anonymity. data analysis the first level of analysis was representational narrative analysis (freeman, 1996; polkinghorne, 1996). in this process the interview data (raw data) were analysed in relation to the research question and represented as a biographical research story for each participant. a thick description of the participant’s context was embedded in the research story. this description allowed the researcher to capture detail, density and depth of the individual experience, thereby allowing the reader the opportunity to feel that he/she understood the experience (charmaz, 1995; plummer, 2001). the stories characterised individuals, described emotions and actions, and located individuals in specific social and temporal contexts. the research story was a good representation device because it integrated the types, processes and configurations of self-identity formations. the task of the researcher was to ensure authentic and fair interpretation of the individual’s experience. it is well recognised that the storied experience is unlikely to mirror the actual experience. therefore, further steps were taken to ensure that the research story represented personal truth as accurately as possible. each participant therefore critiqued his/ her personal research story and suggested changes to make the story a closer representation of their experience. the second level of analysis involved a detailed analysis of research stories into the themes related to the configurations of self-identity formations. the themes were generated through a process of constant comparison between and within cases (miles & huberman, 1994). in this paper excerpts from the research stories (first-level analysis) are combined with the second level thematic analysis across stories. rigour and trustworthiness the rigour and trustworthiness of the study were enhanced through various substantive and ethical validating processes (angen, 2000) applicable to qualitative narrative research. substantive validation was firstly achieved by evaluating the suitability of the methodology in relation to the research question. the choice of life-history methodology was clearly applicable to this study as it was able to generate robust data to understand self-identity formations. secondly, various measures were in place to ensure that the study was credible (lincoln & guba, 1985). credibility of the data collection process was enhanced through in-depth interviewing, member-checking, prolonged engagement, and peer review and debriefing (cresswell & miller, 2000). further to member-checking during the interview process, the research stories were also presented to participants to ensure that their experiences were truthfully represented. peer-review and debriefing was conducted throughout the research process (lincoln & guba, 1985). the researcher, assisted by senior researchers, reflected critically on the process before and after each interview to ensure that her own thoughts and opinions were kept in check. the depth of story telling was enhanced by prolonged engagement (plummer, 2001). in addition to the minimum of three indepth interviews with participants, two additional contacts were made during the analysis process. contact with participants was sustained for approximately a year, allowing the researcher and participants the opportunity for critical reflection and refined data collection. this process also assisted in the development of a trusting relationship, so that sensitive aspects of the story could be probed over time. while triangulation for the purposes of verification (begley, 1996) was not applicable to this study (i.e. personal truth could not be verified through any other method other than personal story-telling), the relevance of triangulation for the purposes of completeness of analysis was considered. the researcher’s analysis of self-identities as the unit of analysis was strengthened by the input of two experienced life-history researchers of different theoretical backgrounds. one expert considered the validity of the analysis of self-identity formations from a disability and impairment perspective, while the other reviewed the plausibility of interpretation from a psychosocial theoretical standpoint. this process assisted in achieving a unit of analysis triangulation (knafl & breitmayer, 1991). ethical validation required that several ethical issues relevant to the methodology were considered. for example, the researcher was cognisant of potential power imbalances between the researcher and participant (kathard, 2009), only used authorised information in research stories and took the necessary steps to ensure that participants’ stories were fairly represented. results and discussion the findings on the configurations of self-identity formations able and disother occurring across the research stories are presented. a brief overview of the configurations within the stories is also provided at the end of the results section. owing to the word limits of the manuscript, excerpts from the stories of only three of these participants are used to illustrate the findings. data excerpts in this paper are from kathard (2003) and also appear in kathard et al. (2004) and kathard (2006). the biographical profiles of participants in the three stories used in this paper are as follows: gareth, a 65-year-old white man, is a retired architect with a university degree. he rated his stuttering as mild at the time of the interview, which correlated with the clinician’s rating. he attended speech therapy weekly for approximately 3 months when he was at primary school. his father and brother were disfluent but were not diagnosed with stuttering. hennie, a 29-year-old white man, is an accountant with a university degree. he rated his stuttering as moderate to severe, which concurred with the clinician’s rating of severity at the time of the interview. hennie had attended the university speech therapy clinic for 6 months at the time of the interview. there was no family history of stuttering. 54 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 self-identity formations of adults who stutter kumari, a 36-year-old indian woman, is an accountant. she rated her stuttering as mild at the time of the interview, which concurred with the clinician’s rating. she had never attended speech therapy. singular self-identity formations at varying points in the stories, the singular self-identity formations (disother or able) were evident. during her early years and throughout adulthood, kumari retained a single, dominant self-understanding of herself as disother (kathard et al., 2004): kumari: i stuttered when i was four. i was detached, isolated and lonely. i had friends, but no best friends because rangini went to another school. my self-esteem was low. rock bottom. i was fashioned out of fear ... i was a fearful, stuttering girl. i gorged on negativity and i couldn’t connect with anyone. the void grew bigger as i grew older ... even if i had a small, simple request like leaving a few minutes early from work, i set myself up for failure. i feel (my abusive father’s) imposing presence. i am a child again, terrified. i stutter, radiate negativity. in contrast, hennie’s story revealed a singular self-identity as being able: hennie: the stuttering was never always such a big problem as it is now. i don’t believe the stuttering, i.e. the speech and the blocks itself, has evolved or changed that much. my father never really punished us. they were easier-going than the rest. i really don’t recall any talk or them being worried about my speech at that time. life was very nice. stuttering was there but i never let it rule my life. i was okay. hennie’s dominant experience of being able occurred in childhood. although he stuttered as a child, stuttering received little negative meaning in his personal and social context. he did not attach negative meaning to stuttering in his early childhood years. therefore, stuttering had little salience and his self-identity as able was constructed on the basis of other positive life experiences. in contrast, kumari’s story during childhood indicated a singular and stable self-identity as disother, created through complex social and personal circumstances over time (kathard et al., 2004). her experience resulted in negative self-definition linked to stuttering as well as her early traumatic experience of abuse. the prominence of a singular negative positioning of self in a world which is dynamic and changing may seem unlikely, yet kumari’s selfidentity as disother had been prevalent for many years of her life. these prolonged experiences of singular negative self-understanding resulted from conservative social and personal forces that act in the interests of minimising complexity, ambivalence and conflict (hermans, 2002). this stable, albeit self-oppressive and rigid knowing of self, is favoured because it fits in with a hierarchical structuring of the world where children, women and people with impairments/disorder occupy lesser positions. in such instances, the self becomes less multi-voiced and more monological. when the disempowered self as disother is reinforced in this monological direction there is a loss of potentiality, and this results in a severe miniaturisation of human beings (sen, 2006), i.e. people have diminished views of who they are. in contrast, hennie’s self-identity as able reveals that it is possible to live with stuttering without being negatively defined by it, as he constructs himself within a discourse of possibility. hennie’s story of positive self-identity is not uncommon in children who may not have internalised understandings of themselves in relation to stuttering. while participants’ self-identities reflect their sense of stability about who they are during their childhood, kumari’s and hennie’s selfunderstandings were very different to each other. further to the singular identity formations, the stories also revealed dual self-identity configurations. dual self-identity configurations co-existing self-identity formations co-existing configurations occurred when both self-identity formations (able and disother) were present in an individual’s story over a particular time period, but were different across contexts. for example, in gareth’s story self-identity as able was prominent on the playground while self-identity as disother was prominent in the classroom, as reported by kathard et al. (2004). gareth in the classroom: at school i never got away with it altogether. the stutter was there and growing. i wanted to forget about it and be like everyone else or better than everyone else. but they wouldn’t let you forget. we hear the dreaded footsteps marching down the corridor. they are two classes away, one class away, and then they are here. the threesome: the principal, the nurse and the school inspector. society watchdogs! we knew what they were looking for, all the misfits, all the problems. maybe they would forget about me. then they start: come to the front when we call your name and problem; mcdonald and hastings – head lice; smith – can’t see well; lovemore – cripple; blake – stutterer. i just want to disappear into the ground. is this all they know about me? hey, remember i came second. they fill out the forms. i remain silent. powerless. they make it unbearable. everyone knows i am a stutterer, but this is a painful public display. gareth in the playground: fortunately, there was always the other side and i gave as good as i got. playground and friends were a real joy. i had a talent and passion for soccer. we get to the field, in teams. captain: gareth blake. coach: gareth blake. manager: gareth blake. i am the team. they played in my team. i hand out the carefully painted badges. i select the team, coach them, talk about the game plan and we play ball. we run, shout, kick and scream. stuttering is furthest from my mind (kathard, 2006). bakhtin (1981) explained that utterances which produce the story of the self come from a particular voice influenced by cultural circumstances and socio-political realities. in society, the power one has is derived in part from social, institutionalised power. for example, there is a relative socially inscribed power that men have over women, boss over worker, and teacher over learner. endowed with socially inscribed power, gareth feels more powerful as a team leader on the playground than in his position as a child who stutters being examined by school authorities. self-identities are contextual and relational, and it is therefore possible to have different understandings of oneself across different contexts (valsiner, 2002). it was possible for the participants to have understandings of themselves as both able and disother – existing fairly separately across contexts. given such fluidity, it is possible to construct self-identity differently across contexts, and therefore to experience stuttering and communication in varied ways across contexts. these multiple selfunderstandings challenge the singular, reductionist discourse promoted by the medical model, which defines the person by the impairment. it emphasises that human lives are plural (sen, 2006), and that there are different ways of living with stuttering. the challenge, even within multidimensional theories of stuttering, is for clinicians and society in general to acknowledge these fluid, multiple, and co-existing selfidentity formations in order to transcend the idea that living with stuttering is necessarily a personal tragedy. competing self-identity formations in the research stories self-identity formations able and disother were sometimes both available in a given context. each self-identity, underpinned by a particular set of beliefs and ideologies, competed for dominance within this context. the competing relationship and dialogue between the self-identities results in a struggle or conflict. contexts and situations are dynamic, and therefore these competing configurations also fluctuate. hennie: imagine this. i walk in the door, tall, blond, macho, strapping; rugby-playing hennie. i am feeling fine. next to me is the guy in the wheelchair. his problem is obvious. i look normal. i am ok for now [feeling able] i open my mouth to speak and … no. the game is not over yet. not over till i stutter. then it happens. out of the blue. it takes me by surprise because i don’t know exactly when it will pop up, and until it does vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 55 self-identity formations of adults who stutter i am normal. you’re not in control of your mouth and that is it. (kathard et al., 2004.) so now a new struggle starts. i have to struggle from being a stutterer to get back to a normal. the only way i can do that is by not stuttering. then that tune plays in my head. don’t stutter. don’t stutter. you battle on. lean over to the normals, from the dark side. lean back from the dark side to the normals, lean back again to the dark side. if you stop and choose silence you lose the race. so i persist. from dark to normal. through all this you just hope that you can make them understand that there is more to this person than just the stutter. i am also another normal behind that abnormal stutter. there is still that normal man with the wit and knowledge to speak his mind. the worst is that sometimes i forget that normal man and then i am the abnormal. abnormal. competing selves present in biographies gave a deeper sense of the complex experience of stuttering. the dialogue between selves, competing for prominence from different ideological positions (salgado & hermans, 2005) created an internal communication fraught with struggle about ‘who am i?’ within a given context. given the dynamic nature of contexts, the selves are constantly positioned and repositioned moment by moment as the communication is negotiated strategically, giving a sense of the effort required in interaction. this ‘unstable equilibrium’ (james, 1982) is an emotionally draining experience, and therefore people must find ways to resolve such instability. viewed differently however, competing selves could be considered as a positive development because this configuration signals that the self has an alternative position, especially when working against a powerful negative self-identity. sen (2006) explained that when the force of a bellicose identity, such as self as disother, is challenged by the competing self-identity, the illusion of a singular, dominant identity is disrupted, allowing recognition of other positive facets of human affiliation and enhancing the possibility of choice. this notion of dual self-identity is not new and aligns with kelly’s (1955) concept of constructive alternativism, i.e. people have alternative ways in which they can reconstruct themselves to enable processes of self-change which has been applied in stuttering intervention by fransella (1968, 1972). however, this study does not only foreground the discrete selfconstructions but importantly the nuanced power struggle between these self-constructions. coalescing self-identity formations in some stories there were harmonising relationships between the seemingly oppositional self-identities at particular points in the story. gareth: through university i was a stutterer and having observed me in the studio presentations they would have said that’s a stutterer. but down the line a different picture emerges. gareth the architect, gareth the father, gareth the grandfather. gareth the stutterer is still there but somewhere in the background. these days i give little attention to my speech and i speak to everyone. i have even become somewhat of a community spokesperson on the forums i sit on. there are just so many other things that occupy my mind now. the self-identities appear to ‘coalesce’, suggesting that there was a harmonising relationship between them and an ‘acceptance’ of the selfidentity as disother. while it seems rather unlikely that a coalition of apparently different selves was possible, the stories suggest that it is. in a study of a ‘coalition of opposites’, bhatia and ram (2001) explain that an understanding of selves must transcend the simplistic pushpull metaphor. the ability to accept the self as both disother and able appears to exist within a symbiotic relationship – one of ambivalence – within a loop that is dynamic in nature. such ambivalence becomes a useful basis for negotiating different selves. in the research stories participants seemed to negotiate living with stuttering successfully when they had accepted stuttering and negotiated their self as disother within their multi-voiced realities. this suggests that being able to bring disparate selves into a harmonising relationship is useful for living successfully with stuttering. configurations within stories although excerpts from the three stories have been chosen to illustrate the different configurations, it must be noted that each story has varied configurations over time. for example, while hennie’s story begins with his self-identity as able, during school and university years and into adulthood he presented both co-existing and competing selfidentity formations. gareth’s self-identity formations changed over his lifespan. while he presented with competing self-identities in different situations in his school life, his experience did not suggest a single dominant negative self-identity at any point. in early adulthood his story revealed competing self-identity formations while in later adulthood he experienced a coalescing of self-identity formations. in contrast, kumari had a self-identity as disother for many years of her life, and it was only in early adulthood that she began to develop her self-identity as able. kumari: i gradually got to view many positive things about myself. mind power became a habit and my new way of living. i learned to trust my logic and intuition and began to recreate myself to become who i want to be. kumari’s competing self-identity formation slowly gained dominance as she gradually reinvented herself in adulthood. conclusion the findings across stories indicated that the configuration of the self-identity formations disother and able occurred in singular and dual configurations. the dual configurations illuminated coexisting, competing, and coalescing relationships between selfidentity formations within and across research stories. these varied configurations highlighted the participants’ subjective, fluid, multiple and complex understandings of themselves as they lived with stuttering over time. while the idea of positive and negative self-identity formations has already been introduced in this and other studies, the complex relationships between the self-identity formations illuminated in this study have important clinical implications. clinical implications life-history interview in assessment for interventions to be personally meaningful, a thorough understanding of the client’s experience of stuttering is necessary. this article described the varied configurations of self-identity formations, using the life-history interview. while it was used in this study as a research method, clinicians could use life-history interviews as a clinical method and engage each client in the personal telling of his/her story in the clinical assessment process. the case-history interview traces the history of stuttering, while the life-history interview invites the client to share how he/she has lived with stuttering in his/her social world over time. by taking this approach the client is co-opted into the assessment process and takes an active role in crafting and sharing his/her story, laying the foundations for participation in his/her own change process. in valuing this subjective/personal truth as legitimate knowledge the clinician gains a fuller empathetic understanding of the client’s reality as a basis for an intervention that is personally meaningful (hagstrom & wertsch, 2004). the techniques for conducting a life-history interview are described in detail in various life-history methodology texts. applied to stuttering, the following strategies were found to be useful (kathard, 2009): • at the outset of the assessment the clinician must reassure the client that the intention of sharing his/her story is not to assess the frequency of stuttering or observe his/her struggle, but rather to hear the details about the experiences he/she thinks are most relevant. however, that he/she may block or struggle is likely and there should be an open negotiation about how communication breakdown on 56 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 self-identity formations of adults who stutter the part of the listener and speaker (e.g. long silences, interruptions, loss of eye contact) will be managed. • given that the intention is to hear the client’s experience, the client has the freedom to select the issues he/she wants to talk about, in terms of events and critical moments which shaped his/her life most. the task of the clinician is to offer a non-directive space in which to generate the story and to listen deeply and non-judgementally. while the clinician can probe aspects of the story, he/she should not redirect the course of storytelling during the initial phase. • the process may appear innocent and non-threatening, but it has potential to excavate truths that the client has masked or obliterated which may be unpleasant for him/her. the clinician should therefore be vigilant and proceed cautiously, allowing the client to share what he/she is comfortable with initially and other aspects of the story may emerge later in the process. for example, a person who experienced abuse as a child who stuttered may not talk about it at the initial stages of the process but may do so later when a secure relationship has been established with the clinician. the story may also cause issues to surface which are beyond the clinician’s scope of practice and therefore warrant referral to a relevant source. • some clients have difficulty telling a story and they can be assisted through reflective diary keeping, free writing and including artefacts (letters, poems, drawings). in a multilingual context, clients should be provided with opportunities to share their stories in the language/s in which they feel most comfortable, allowing them the freedom to use informal language, gesture, code mixing and switching to enhance their story telling. some clients are not candidates for this type of narrative methodology and therefore the clinician needs to decide on its use on a case-by-case basis. • the clinician has the task of placing the story within a larger sociocultural context and should probe the temporal and social contexts during which critical events occurred. for example, the stories in this study were shared by participants who lived through apartheid in south africa and their experiences as people who stuttered were also shaped by their broader social reality. for some, discrimination was not experienced only as a consequence of stuttering, but also by virtue of their race. some participants reported their experiences during a particular era when schools took the view that children should be ‘seen and not heard’ and illuminated how this reality shaped their experience of stuttering. by listening to the story, the clinician should be able to identify the patterns of self-identity formations evident in the client’s story. for example, are the two formations able and disother evident in the story? when do they occur in the story – across context and time; which is the dominant self-identity strand? this mapping would allow the clinician to interpret the client’s behaviour as part of his/her selfidentity formation, i.e. what he/she does is related to who he/she is in a particular context. for example, if a client has a self-identity as disother in a classroom, he/she may be silent in an attempt to pass for normal – his/her behaviour therefore is part of his identity management. a mapping of the self-identity formations will also assist the clinician in targeting the contexts for intervention and appreciating that different strategies may be necessary across contexts. understanding the power dynamics this study illuminated that while the individual can have both positive and negative understandings of themselves, when in a competing relationship they cause issues of conflict, power and ambivalence to surface, i.e. political dimension. this power struggle within and against oneself speaks to a layer of struggle that is an inherent part of the experience. the client must therefore be empowered to negotiate his/ her own internal power struggle as part of a repertoire of skills to enable the desired intervention outcome. when using self-identity as a lens the clinician is obliged to ask questions such as: which of the self-identities is dominant and why? how can the ambivalence between self-identities be creatively managed? how can the relationship between self-identity formations shift from a competing to harmonising relationship? narrative intervention this study was not intervention-based. however, narrative approaches to intervention may be useful in facilitating self-change in adults who stutter. the methodology is derived from constructivist theory based on the assumption that people have the ability to deconstruct and reconstruct themselves to create new realities. the advantage of the narrative approach is that the client can create a personally meaningful intervention by connecting his/her past with his/her future by determining the desired outcomes and realities within his/her range of competence (biggs & hinton-bayre, 2008). the alternative story is seen as an opportunity for the client to gain greater control of their lives by constructing different views of themselves and taking the necessary actions to achieve a new vision. there are different ways of achieving the alternative story. for example, polkinghorne (1996) explained that clients described life plots that were agentic or victimic. in the victimic plot, which is similar to self-identity as disother, a person constructed him/herself in a negative way and was characterised by blame and lack of progressive movement. in contrast, the agentic life plot was one in which the person was actively engaged in advancing towards an attainable goal. the task of the clinician was to assist the client in uniting all the elements of his/her life into a story that reveals his/her identity and actions and then to challenge and confront the victimic plot through a process by which he/she detaches him/herself from the story as he/she creates a new story coupled with relevant action. dilollo et al. (2002) used narrative therapy in stuttering intervention, based on the rationale that therapy outcomes are more likely to be successful and sustainable when the self is deconstructed from the stuttering-dominated self to the self that takes on the fluent speaker role. they provide a useful narrative methodology to achieve this. appreciation of fuller self-identity in contrast to dilollo et al. (2002), who focus on the fluent speaker role in the self-reconstruction process, the authors of this paper offer different perspectives on the reconstruction process and fluent speaker role. self-construction/reconstruction does not limit the able identity only to a fluent speaking role. the able identity (kathard, 2006) is crafted from a more all-embracing positive sense of self which includes the role as better communicator and fluent speaker but is not limited to it. the clinical implication is therefore that while fluency and improved communication are part of a positive self-identity there are other personal successes like being in a position of power, successful at work and relationships that empower individuals and drive a positive change. the clinician should therefore capitalise on an asset-based approach by focusing on strengths to build on. this perspective offers the possibility of constructing and acknowledging a more complete able self-identity and using it as a catalyst to drive further positive self-construction including that of good communicator/fluent speaker. the self as able is more robust and resilient and therefore not easily disrupted or dislodged by episodes of stuttering as the person has a wide-ranging resource base of skills including communication skills. furthermore, while other studies promote fluency and attainment of a self-concept as a fluent speaker as the end goal of intervention, this study suggests that there are different ways of living positively with impairment. self as disother and able could coalesce – allowing the idea of one within the other – a coalition of opposites. it is suggestive of a compassionate living of different selves, acknowledging that disother is part of one’s story but not the only story. finally, the findings have implications for relapse. while fluency has traditionally been used as an indicator of relapse, perkins (1979) argued that lapses in self-identity as a stutterer would be a more robust indicator than the narrow confines of maintaining fluent speech. in agreement with perkins, the authors of this paper tentatively suggest vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 57 self-identity formations of adults who stutter that a lapse or a dislodging of self-identity as able and the resurgence and strengthening, of self-identity as disother, together with negative identity management strategies e.g. avoidance, or concealing stuttering to pass for normal, are other indicators of relapse. therefore, the aim of interventions should focus on strengthening self-identity as a basis for improved communication. focus of change: self and society while intervention efforts focus on the individual as the locus of change, this study also points to the powerful social system which constructs stuttering in a negative light. people who stutter live in environments which have negative views of stuttering and hold general negative stereotypes of stuttering (blood, blood, tellis & gabel, 2003). society plays a critical role in shaping both positive and negative selfidentities. while we might continue to work with the individual, the clinician is equally challenged to change the attitudes of society to become accepting of people who stutter for who they are – as people (who stutter) – as part of a continuum of diversity. acknowledgements we thank the participants for generously sharing their stories and express our gratitude to the national research foundation (south africa) for supporting the project financially. references angen, m. j. (2000). evaluating interpretive inquiry: reviewing the validity debate and opening the dialogue. qualitative health research, 10(3), 378-395. bakhtin, m. m. (1981). the dialogical imagination. austin, tx: university of texas press. begley, c. m. (1996). using triangulation in nursing research. journal of advanced nursing, 24, 122-128. bhatia, s., & ram, a. (2001). acculturation, dialogical voices and the construction of the diasporic self. commentary: culture and psychology, 7, 297-309. biggs, h. c., & hinton-bayre, a. d. (2008). telling tales to end wails: narrative therapy techniques and rehabilitation counseling. australian journal of rehabilitation counselling, 14(1), 16-25. blood, g. w., blood, i. m., tellis, g. m., & gabel, r. m. (2003). a preliminary study of selfesteem, stigma, and disclosure in adolescents who stutter. journal of fluency disorders, 28, 143-159. boehmer, e. (1995), colonial and postcolonial literature. oxford: oxford university press. charmaz, k. (1995). grounded theory. in j. a. smith, r. harre & l. van langenhove (eds.), rethinking methods in psychology (pp. 2749). london: sage. clandinin, d. j., & connelly, f. m. (1994). personal experience methods. in n. k. denzin & y. lincoln (eds.), handbook of qualitative research (pp. 413-427). thousand oaks, ca: sage. cresswell, j. w., & miller, d. l. (2000). determining validity in qualitative inquiry. theory into practice, 39(3), 124-130. dilollo, a., niemeyer, r. a., & manning w. h. (2002). a personal construct view of relapse: indications for a narrative component to stuttering treatment. journal of fluency disorders, 27, 19-42. dilollo, a., & wolter, j. (2004). qualitative research in communication disorders. the asha leader, 9 (june 11), 4-5, 16-17. evesham, m., & fransella, f. (1985). stuttering relapse: the effect of combined speech and psychological reconstruction programme. british journal of disorders of communication, 20, 237-248. fransella, f. (1968). self-concepts and the stutterer. british journal of psychiatry, 114, 15311535. fransella, f. (1972). stuttering: not a symptom but a way of life. international journal of language and communication disorder, 5(1), 22-29. freeman, d. (1996). to take them at their word: language data in the study of teacher’s knowledge. harvard educational review, 66(4), 732-761. hagstrom f., & wertsch, j. v. (2004). grounding social identity for professional practice. topics in language disorders, 24(4), 162-173. hatch, j. a., & wisniewski, r. (1995). life history and narrative: questions, issues and exemplary works. in j. a. hatch & r. wisniewski (eds.), life history and narrative (pp. 113-137). london: falmer press. hermans, h. j. m. (2002). the dialogical self as a society of mind: introduction. theory & psychology, 12(2), 147-160. james, w. (1982). the varieties of religious experience. a study in human nature (gifford lectures in natural religion delivered at edinburgh, 1901-1902). new york: penguin press. kathard, h. (2003). life histories of people who stutter: on becoming someone. unpublished doctoral thesis, university of durban-westville, kwazulu-natal, south africa. kathard, h. (2006). on becoming someone. self-identity as able: life histories of people who stutter. advances in speech-language pathology, 8(2), 79-91. kathard, h. (2009). personal truth making. a cautious celebration. in r. dhanpath & m. samuel (eds.), life history research: epistemology, methodology and representation (pp. 1836). rotterdam: sense publishers. kathard, h., pillay, m., samuel, m., & reddy, v. (2004). genesis of self-identity as disother. south african journal of communication disorders, 41, 3-14. kelly, g. a. (1955). the psychology of personal constructs (vol i & ii). norton. knafl, k. a., & breitmayer, b. j. (1991). triangulation in qualitative research issues of conceptual clarity and purpose. in j. m. morse (ed.), qualitative nursing research: a contemporary dialogue (pp. 226-239). thousand oaks, ca: sage. lincoln, y. s., & guba, e. g. (1985). naturalistic inquiry. thousand oaks, ca: sage. measor, l., & sikes, p. (1992). visiting lives: ethics and methodology in life history. in i. f. goodson (ed.), studying teachers’ lives (pp. 209-233). new york: teachers college press. miles, m. b., & huberman, a. m. (1994). qualitative data analysis: an expanded sourcebook (2nd ed.). thousand oaks, ca: sage. mishler, e. g. (1999). storylines: craftartists’ narratives of identity. london: harvard university press. perkins, w. h. (1979). from psychoanalysis to discoordination. in h. h. gregory (ed.), controversies about stuttering therapy (pp. 97-127). baltimore, md: university park press. petrunik, m., & shearing, c. d. (1983). fragile façade: stuttering and the strategic manipulation of awareness. social problems, 31(2), 125-136. plummer, k. (2001). documents of life 2: an invitation to critical humanism. london: sage. polkinghorne, d. e. (1996). explorations of narrative identity. psychological inquiry, 7, 363367. rubin, h. j., & rubin, i. (1995). qualitative interviewing: the art of hearing data. london: sage. salgado, j., & hermans, h. j. m. (2005). the return of subjectivity: from a multiplicity of selves to the dialogical self. e-journal of applied psychology: clinical section, 1(1), 3-13. http://ojs. lib.swin.edu.au/index.php/ejap. samuel, m. (2009). on becoming a teacher: life history research and the force-field model of teacher development. in r. dhanpath & m. samuel (eds.), life history research: epistemology, methodology and representation (pp. 1-16). rotterdam: sense publishers. sen, a. (2006). identity and violence: the illusion of destiny. london: ww norton & co. shearer, w. m. (1961). a theoretical consideration of the self-concept and body image in stuttering therapy. american speech & hearing association, 3, 115-116. sheehan, j. g. (1970). stuttering: research and theory. new york: harper. silverman, e. (2001, october). consumer alert: stuttering and gender research. paper presented at 4th international stuttering awareness day conference (on-line conference). retrieved from http://www.mnsu.edu/dept/comradis/isad4/papers/silverman2.html somers, m. r. (1994). the narrative constitution of identity: relational and network approach. theory and society: renewal and critique in social theory, 23(5), 604-635. stewart, t., & birdsall, m. (2001). a review of the contribution of personal construct psychology to stammering therapy. journal of constructivist psychology, 14, 215-226. tetnowski, j. a., & damico, j. s. (2001). a demonstration of the advantages of qualitative methodologies in stuttering research. journal of fluency disorders, 26(1), 17-42. valsiner, j. (2002). forms of dialogical relations and semiotic autoregulations within the self. theory & psychology, 12(2), 251-265. journal of the south african logopedic society june chronaxy measurement of the nervus recurrens and its applications by g. h. breckwoldt, ph. d. department of phonetics and logopedics, university of the witwatersrand. the various experiments, briefly outlined in the previous number of this journal (cf. journal of the s.a. logopedic society, vol. 4, no. 2, p.10. seq.) led r. husson to measure "in situ" the excitability of the n. recurrens of man and to discuss the practical uses of his measurements, (cf. r. husson: "la mesure 'in situ' de l'excitabilite recurrentielle chez l'homme et ses applications . . . ", bulletin de l'academie nat. de med., no.s 1 et 2, 1955.) husson says that moulonguet's experiments, which proved that there exists a congruity between the action potentials of the n. recurrens and the vibrations of the vocal folds, has made it possible to measure the chronaxy of the recurrent nerve. he works out a simple basic formula to determine the maximum frequency: 1000 maximum frequency = ν == . (r. hussonκ c ibid., p. 1) in this formula c is the chronaxy of the recurrent nerve in milliseconds, and its refractory period lasts κ chronaxies. this formula makes it possible to calculate the highest note a subject can produce, if (through some method or other) one knows the chronaxy of his recurrent nerve. conversely, the highest note sung by a subject makes it possible to calculate his chronaxy. there is another important finding worth mentioning in this connection: the chronaxy which is calculated from the n. recurrens is the same as the one which is easily measured on the motor point of the sternocleidomastoid muscle. husson describes the simple method of measuring the chronaxy: — the subject holds in his hand the anode electrode, and the examiner applies the cathode electrode to the motor point of the m. sternocleidomastoideus. the study of just over 100 subjects, mostly singers with well trained and well established voices, made it possible for husson to draw up a table, which gives the values of the recurrent chronaxy for each type of grown-up voice, i.e. from the ultra high soprano of chronaxic value 0.055 to the deepest bass of chronaxic value 0.170. the following table shows the correlation between the measurements of n. recurrens chronaxy and the voice classification of singing voices: chronaxy values male voice in milliseconds female voice very high tenor middle tenor low tenor intermediary voice intermediary voice high baritone middle baritone deep baritone intermediary voice intermediary voice high lyric bass deep lyric bass middle bass middle bass deep bass deep bass 0.055 0.060 0.065 0.070 0.075 0.080 0.085 0.090 0.095 0.100 0.105 0.110 0.115 0.120 0.130 0.140 0.150 0.160 0.170 ultra-high soprano ultra-high soprano very high soprano high soprano middle soprano low soprano , intermediary voice high mezzo soprano middle mezzo soprano deep mezzo soprano intermediary voice high mezzo contralto middle mezzo contralto deep mezzo contralto intermediary voice intermediary voice contralto contralto contralto (r. husson, ibid., p.6) r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) june journal of the south african logopedic society this table shows that for each sex there are not only 3 or 4 types of voice, but that there is an infinity of voice types, one leading into the other. all chronaxic values are possible between 0.055, and 0.170. many voices, therefore are intermediary among the classical types of voice. this explains the difficulty some singers have to fall into one particular category; i.e. with regard to their innate voice range. chronaxic values only determine pitch, but in no way intensity or timbre. looking at husson's table, one might be surprised at finding that men and women have identical excitability of the n. recurrens and consequently the same chronaxic values. a man and a woman of identical recurrent chronaxies present a voice range with a difference of exactly one octave. this difference is due to the fact that the man uses a monophasic recurrent conduction (chest register), whereas the woman uses a biphasic recurrent conduction (head register). nevertheless a man can make use of his biphasic register in high tones (falsetto voice), but he cannot keep it up continuously without experiencing fatigue. a woman can also use her monophasic register in deep tones, but not for a prolonged time without feeling fatigued. research has also been done on the voices of children, and e. j. garde and husson found that in children, aged 8-15 years, whom they examined, the chronaxies were ranging from 0.060 to 0.160, i.e. exactly as in adults. it would be interesting to know, if the recurrent chronaxy before and after pubertal voice change remains the same or undergoes changes. the publications i have read do not contain any information on this subject. research in this field might give revealing results. on the other hand, research has been carried out to show that certain medications influence the chronaxy of the recurrent nerva (e.g. the diminishing effect of thyroxine). a change in the recurrent chronaxy, which is of particular interest to the logopedician, is the one which has been observed in cases of phonasthenia. husson writes about his findings in this field and says that in phonasthenia the change in recurrent chronaxy is most noticeable. in each case one always finds, simultaneously: 1. a decrease of the rheobase; 2. an increase of the chronaxy. in addition to this most important observation, there is another research result of the laboratory of physiology of the sorbonne, which especially concerns the logopedician: — it has been found in phonasthenia that 1. the dyschronaxy, which appears, is always unilateral; 2. the affected vocal fold is always the more "controlled" one (which means the right fold in right-handed persons, the left fold in left'handed ones). an exception to this rule has not been found. husson gives a couple of examples of cases of phonasthenia, showing the obvious correlation between handedness and recurrent chronaxy: subject recurrent chronaxy right left miss b., aged 34, left-handed 0.098 0.165 miss d., aged 21, right-handed 0.072 0.061 miss m., aged 24, right-handed 0.151 0.104 master p., aged 6, left-handed 0.109 0.132 mrs. s., aged 42, right-handed ... 0.320 0.098 (r. husson, ibid., p.6) huson observed that as phonasthenia progresses the chronaxy of both vocal folds increases, but it always remains raised in the fold which is the more "controlled" one (cf. handedness). if phonasthenia cases are given suitable medication or only a period of strict vocal rest, one will see the following results: with the compliments of: messrs. a. loewenthal metals (pty.) ltd. 13, maraisburg road industria west johannesburg or p.o. box 6546 telephone 35-6744 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society june 1. increase of .the rheobase; 2. decrease of the chronaxy, descending to its •·"•• normal physiological level. the value of these findings to the logopedician is evident. even the latest laryngo-stroboscope does not give the examiner as revealing a result as the measuring of the recurrent chronaxy. laryngo-stroboscopic examination may reveal very clearly a difference in amplitude in the vibration of both vocal folds, but never gives any information whether the amplitude is e.g. abnormally increased in the one fold or abnormally decreased in the other. a chronaxic measurement dispels all doubt: the vocal trouble is situated in that side which has the higher recurrent chronaxy. the logopedician will be able to adiapt his therapy accordingly. he may, now, find himself confronted with new therapeutic demands. this does not mean in any way that previous therapeutic approaches like relaxation and breathing exercises should be abandoned. on the contrary, in addition to therapy which in the past, has proved successful, we may, now, be able to . develop measures based on chronaxic measurement and thus create a more intensive, possibly more speedy, therapy. in the above i have tried to give a brief description of the researches, which have taken place at the sorbonne and deal wath aspects of voice physiology which have caused an upheaval in the scientific world. husson has a great number of followers among the leading figures of phonetic science. one of the first and foremost personalities, who have subscribed to husson's new theories, is g. panconcellicalzia. his publication "breathing in phonation, new and old aspects" (die stimmatmung, das neue, das alte), leipzig, 1956, is based on hijsson's ideas. putting things in a nutshell, calzia states in this book that: "the motto of the classical old italian school of singing was: 'chi ben respira, ben canta.' (who breathes well, sings well). that was wrong. it would have been more correct to say: 'chi ben canta, ben respira', because husson has proved that the activities, in the larynyx and pharynx contract the breathing and not vice versa. in spite of these new findings some phoneticians and logopedicians still adhere to the old historical sentimentality." (calzia, die stimmatmung, p. 14). in a recent publication we still find an enthusiastic adherence to what calzia calls "the old historical sentimentality." i am referring to the book "the voice and its disorders" by margaret greene, london, 1957, which i reviewed in the previous number of this journal (cf. vol. 4, no. 2, p. 15, seq.). the author characteristically calls a chapter, which deals with the breathing mechanism, "the vocal excitor" (cf. m. greene, ibid., ch.2). husson's ideas have not been accepted everywhere, and it appears that some scientists, who repeated his experiments, have not come to the same conclusions. . during an international symposium on the psysiology of the larynx, organized by the french society of phoniatrics, held in paris in october, 1955, some scientists, e.g. a. fessard and b. vallancien, tried to disprove some of husson's and laget's experiments. they came to the conclusion that their experiments were incompatible with the theory that the frequency of vocal fold vibration is neurogenically determined, (cf. folia phoniatrica* 1957, vol. 9, no. 1, p.62, seq., and ibid., 1957, vol. 9, no. 3, p.152, seq.). the danish scientist k. faaborg-andersen comes to similar conclusions in his experiments, (cf. acta physiologioa scandinavian 41, supplementum 140, 1957). in june, -1957 a high speed film was shown at baden-baden, which also disproved some of husson's findings, i.e. vocal fold-vibration and phonation only occurred, when there -was a subglottal air supply, (cf. personal correspondence with r. luchsinger, zurich, june, 1957.) long before husson published his experimental results, i had theoretically assumed that the vooal folds are centrally "steered" or initiated. to this conclusion one comes most logically, if one is familiar with the integrative action of the neuro-muscular system*. husson is no doubt right in his idea of neimrchtonaxy of the vocal folds. one cannot help visualizing the recurrent chronaxy acting like an electric motor on the phonation process. but in all acts of normal phonation subglottal functions must be taken into consideration as much as laryngeal functions. the synthesis of infra-laryngeal, laryngeal and supra-laryngeal functions makes up normal phonation. it would be a misjudging of the integration and synchronization of psycho-physical functions to say that the vocal mechanism modifies (or even steers) the breathing or, vice versa, that the breathing modifies the voice functions. in the normal individual the balanced interaction of all functions is the standard. in an abnormal situation we get an unbalanced interaction. if a person is suddenly thrown into an unusual emotional upheaval, his voice will be affected and so will be his breathing. the same applies in an abnormal physical situation. if a person has to run two miles without stopping, his breathing will • it should be remembered that both nerve and muscle are developed from previously undifferentiated neuromuscular tissue (cf. c. l. meader and j. h. muyskens: handbook of biolinguistics, toledo, 1950, iv, p.57:) "research on sobbing might prove interesting in this connection. in the spasmodic interaction of diaphragm and larynx in sobbing it. is impossible to allot a predominant role toeither functioni r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) june journal of the south african logopedic society be affected, and, in trying to speak or sing, his voice will be equally affected. we cannot separate breathing and voice in phonation**. they are both neurogenic and act upon each other, whatever occasional time-lag in synchronization may occur. whether we accept husson's ideas or not, his publications are very stimulating; they invite a repetition of his experiments and further researches in the field of normal and abnormal voice production, e.g. voice studies during puberty, male falsettos, hysterical dysphonics and aphonics and subliminal phonation. it is to be expected that the chronaxic measurement of the recurrent nerve will not remain, a study for research only, but may become as much a routine examination in the practice of the logopedician and phoniatrician as laryngoscopy and laryngostroboscopy. it can serve as a periodic examination to check up on the progress of therapy. it may lead the way e.g. to a new type of specific relaxation therapy, which will take the question of hand dominance into consideration. bibliograhpy amado, j. h.: rapport a.f.e.p.l., 16-17 oct. 1952, annates d' oto-laryng., 1953, 70. no.s 2-3. amado, j. h.: rev. laryng. 1954, suppl ffevr. 284. dumont, p.: etude chronaxim6trique sur le larynx, thfese, paris, 1933. eyrifes, ch..: encyclop. mid.-chlrurg., vol. o.r.l., 20623 a 10, paris, nov., .1955. faaborg-andersen, k : acto physiologlca scandinavia, • ' • '41, supplementum 140, 1957. fessard and vailancien: fol. phon., 1957, 9/1, 63, and ibid., 1967, 9/3, 152, seq. garde, e. j.: la voix, collection "que sals-je?", presses univ. france, idit., paris, 1954. goerttler, k . : zeitschr. f. anat. u. entwickl., bd. 116 1950. 352-401, 36 flg. greene, m. c. l.: the voice and its disorders, pitman med. publ. co., london, 1964. gutzmann, h.: sprachheilkunde, berlin, 1924. hildernesse, l.' w . : logop. en phon., 1956, 28/1,2,3. hlldernesse and husson: logop. en phon., 1956, 28/10, 11, 12, and 1957, 29/3. husson, r.: fol. phon., 1952, 3/4, 240. husson, r. and chenay, c.: c. r. acad. sciences, 1953, 236, 1.077. husson, r.:' bulletin de l'acadim. nat. de mid., no. 1 & 2, 1955. husson, r.:' encyclop. mid.-chirurg., vol. o.r.l., 20623 β 10, paris, nov., 1955. krmpotic, j.: rev. laryng., portmann, suppl. de f<5vr., 1957. ' " laget, p.: j . physiol., 1953, 45, 147. meader and muyskens: handbook of biolinguistlcs, toledo, 1950. moulonguet, α.: rev. laryng, tivr., 1954, suppl., 110127. panconcelli-calzia, g.: die stimmatung, das neue, das alte, leipzig, 1956. portmann, humbert, robin, laget, husson: c.r. soc. biol., paris 12 fevr., 1955. 149, 269. ranson and clark: the anatomy of the nervour system, saunders, london, 8th ed., 1947. winckel, f.: funk und ton, 1953, iii, 124-132. westdene products (pty.) ltd. 23 essanby house 175 jeppe street johannesburg. (branches in cape town, durban & pretoria) are specialists in medical literature. we recommend: language for the pre school deaf child — by grace harris lassman, teacher of the deaf, formerly instructor of speech, john tracy clinic, los angeles, u.s.a. price 51/the development and disorders of speech in childhood — by muriel e. morley, speech therapist-in-charge of the speech therapy unit, the united newcastleupon tyne teaching hospitals. price 52/6. ("this book presents a classification of development disorders of speech. the findings relating to various types of speech disorder are given and principles of treatment described.") artistic bookbinders braamfontein — phone 44-6584 21 stiemens street: transvalia buildings shop 8 in mell street. how important are your φ journals of speech and hearing disorders φ "speech" φ notes ι these can be bound in attractive volumes to suit your needs. we specialise in binding your thesis according to your design and colour scheme to insure your success. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) some observations on group work with adult stammerers by r. fawcus l.c.s.t. m. a. fawcus l.c.s.t. organisation of the groups. the london county council has for many years held evening classes for adult stammerers. until 1956, general relaxation was a fundamental part of treatment. the present approach dates from this period, when one of us was appointed as a speech therapist and "non-avoidance" techniques were introduced. the classes are now affiliated to the city literary institute, which holds a wide variety of classes in the arts and the humanities. students from the age of 16 years enrol in the evening groups for adult stammerers with no more formality than the completion of a short form, and the payment of a fee of 25 shillings. this fee entitles them to attend twice weekly over the entire academic year (a period of nine months from october to june). there are now three therapists, each taking two groups a week, and groups are now available four nights a week. those stammerers who come twice weekly (approximately 50% of the total this year) seldom see the same therapist on both evenings. we feel that this policy not only helps to sustain their interest, but helps them to gain a broader view of the problem. facilities for individual discussion are always made available when necessary, particularly on a thursday evening, when two therapists are present both before and after the regular class session. more often than not on this evening, one or two students stay on for an informal discussion after the class, or else come in early for a talk. we find very few stammerers requiring regular individual help. little or no attempt is made to "screen" stammerers who want to join the classes, and the main factor determining which group or groups the student joins will be the particular evening on which he wants to come. this results in a fairly random selection, with a present age range of 16 to 55 years. in addition to this wide age range, there are varied racial, social and educational backgrounds. this year we have students from ceylon, ghana, india, ireland, libya, nigeria, south africa and the west indies. nonetheless, the common problem of stammering makes this a homogenous group in the psychotherapeutic sense. a total of 50 students have enrolled since the end of september: 28 of the total enrolment are new students, and the remainder are in their second or third year. these are "open groups" in the sense used by anthony (1957) since students are enrolling at intervals throughout the year. it is advocated that the homogenous group should be run on closed lines, but not only would this be impossible in practice, but we believe there is a positive value in exposing the stammerers to the stress both of new members and visitors (such as speech therapy students) joining the group. whilst our attendance varies from 7-13 students, our experience confirms anthony's statement that 8 is the "ideal" number for a therapeutic group. origins and rationale of our approach. working with groups of stammerers over a period of 6 years has led to a growing conviction that the stammerer is basically a normal speaker, with normal and understandable speech fears, sometimes resulting in withdrawal, sometimes in aggression, but all within the range of normal human behaviour. dean williams, considering research findings on stammering comments: "according to present day knowledge, it appears most reasonable to conclude that those persons who come to be classified by themselves or 7 by others as stutterers are not distinctive from other persons, either neuro-physiologically or either in respect to basic personality structure". from our experience, the really neurotic stammerer stands out in a group by his persistent resistance to treatment, by his failure to become an integrated part of the group, or by other abnormal patterns of social behaviour. evidence seems to suggest that the incidence of neurosis 16 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) amongst stammerers is much the same as that among the population as a whole. the question of whether stammering is or is not a symptom of neurosis is, of course, a perennially controversial one. one tends, naturally, to interpret evidence according to what one believes, but we are increasingly impressed by the essentially normal behaviour of stammerers. we have therefore based our approach to the problem on the assumption that the stammerer is a "normal" person with a normal speech mechanism, and that the stammering can be considered as a functionally autonomous disorder of communication. qualitatively and quantitatively he has the same kind of emotional problems as the normal speaker, plus those anxieties arising secondary to the stammer. it is in fact, a functional speech disorder in the purest sense. physical symptoms, such as biochemic, cardiac and respiratory changes which have been noted in the extensive literature on the subject are again held to be the effect rather than the cause of stammering, since none of these seem to be incompatible with either the symptoms of fear experienced by a normal speaker or with what one can see and hear the stammerer doing when he stammers. to illustrate our point with an example: there are still therapists, particularly in europe, who set out to re-educate the stammerer's breathing pattern on the basis of their findings of abnormal pneumographic recordings. during so-called "normal" or fluent speech, we have found only one or two stammerers who showed a sufficiently abnormal breathing pattern to interfere with speech in any way. yet during stammering the majority of stammerers will exhibit an abnormal pattern for any one or more of the following reasons: (a) because he wants to say as much as he can on a single breath (to avoid the "difficulties" of stopping and starting again), so that he continues speaking on residual air. (b) because he makes an "anticipatory inspiration", an habitual reaction to a feared word which probably has its roots in the mistaken advice to take a deep breath by a well-meaning parent, teacher or friend, or because he feels he needs a good breath force to break through his block! (c) during a clonic stammer, there will be an abnormal expiratory pattern, as air is released during the tremor. (d) in a tonic block, air is held in the lungs under pressure beneath the level of a closed glottis. to give re-education exercises is wholly to miss the point and entirely misinterpret these symptoms. all that is required is for these to be pointed out to him, and for him to understand how the first two increase the overt abnormality, and the last two are the direct result of what he is doing with his vocal cords or muscles of articulation. as stammering decreases, so these "symptoms" of abnormal breathing decrease or disappear. if breathing exercises are claimed to be effective, then it is important to realise that this is either due to the effects of suggestion, where the stammerer's conviction of their efficiency overcomes his fear of stammering (which is highly unlikely) or where, in concentrating on his breathing, they serve as a distraction device. the block can obviously be eliminated if the stammerer is really thinking so hard about his breathing, and how to control it, that he is too busy to do the other things which normally interfere with his speech. this interference will be discussed in a later section. basically, our approach is two-fold: the first and most important is to change attitude towards the stammer; the second is to bring about change and modifications in the abnormal speech behaviour. in practice it is found that work on changing one always affects and re-inforces the other. without a profound change in attitude towards stammering, little can be expected in the way of progress. this is why we feel that therapies directed towards achieving speech fluency without adequate work on attitude is bound to have limited or transient effect. it is easy enough to achieve fluency by various means in the clinical situation through general relaxation, suggestion and distraction techniques. emphasis tends to be placed on fluency as a criterion of progress. the stammerer is greatly encouraged by this fluency, but it is based on the flimsiest of foundations, and he is correspondingly depressed and discouraged, when under stress conditions he blocks as severely as before. we december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) could have learned a great deal more about stammering if it had really been understood why methods had apparently succeeded which had no business to succeed at all! so often the prestige of the therapist, distraction, and suggestion generally, were responsible in cases where the techniques, methods or exercises used have been given the credit. this would not matter if we could rely on such therapies to be successful with most stammerers and also for the effects to be lasting. one can well understand why demosthenes' pebbles could have been successful! we may imagine the distraction afforded by trying to speak with a mouthful of pebbles, and the concentration required not to swallow them! in our present day, one can appreciate the strong suggestion involved when diagnosis and treatment are carried out under extremely clinical conditions, with remedial exercises, hypnotherapy, sedation, investigations into respiratory, cardiac and nervous function, etc. the stammerer becomes convinced that his stammering is a manifestation of a physical abnormality, if he did not already believe this to be so. the same thing seems to occur after extensive psychotherapy, particularly psychoanalysis, where the patient becomes convinced that his stammering is symptomatic of some deep emotional conflict. the stammerer echoes "i am convinced that if i could find the cause, my stammering would be cured." many causes are found for him, some he will accept, others he will reject. this often prevents his looking objectively at his stammering behaviour as something he is doing — both to interfere with speech and to increase the abnormality of his behaviour. as dean williams says, "as long as one functions as though an 'it' makes things happen, he is not motivated to observe cause and effect relationships in his behaviour, for 'it' is both cause and effect". further, "he talks and acts as though he believes either that there exists a little man inside him eager to grab certain words, or that certain words are possessed of physical properties such that they get stuck in his throat". we are basically in agreement with the semantogenic approach to stammering (johnson 1957). we feel, however, that a full consideration of this theory is not within the scope of this paper. we profess to an eclectic philosophy, drawing our information from fields related and unrelated to speech pathology. the psycho-physiology of stammering. when an individual stammers we are confronted by several different categories of behaviour. when he anticipates difficulty we may observe avoidance of a specific word, rearrangement of words or a total avoidance of speech. some will interject a sound, word or phrase whilst others will resort to such activities as taking a breath or swallowing to delay blocking. this behaviour is almost always initiated consciously and the stammerer is usually well aware of this fact. some will complain, however, that it happens so quickly that it is apparently automatic. the actual blocking or repetition appears to represent a loss of conscious volitional control. we would suggest that this is mainly due to a lack of feedback of information, made worse by the stammerer's emotional state. we would not consider that his general tension, however severe, is the cause of blocking. it is by exerting specific laryngeal tension that he directly interferes in the process of phonation, either disrupting the production of voice or preventing it altogether. the laryngeal activity clearly described by kenyon (1943) has been largely ignored by most workers. not only can one observe glottal closure or clonus on vowels and diphthongs, but an identical action occuring on both voiced and voiceless consonants. we are all familiar with the phenomenon of the distorted voiced plosive which a stammerer may produce as an ejective: e.g. p'read and p'utter. another common feature is the emission of a constricted flow of air which will permit the utterance of a voiceless sound but does not involve the vibration necessary for a voiced consonant or following vowel. one can imitate these phenomena by holding one's breath as lightly as possible and then "trying" to phonate. further information may be gained by employing a throat microphone with a suitable amplifier and comparing the sounds resulting from both geuine arid assumed stammering. our observations would tend to confirm ; the conclusions reached by dean williams (1955) in his electromyographic studies, namely that there was no significant difference between action potentials produced by stammerers and those 1 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) by normal speakers simulating stammering. the stammerer undergoes all the normal physical manifestations of a state of fear, which he identifies as "feelings of stammering". these are, however, easily recognised in most speech therapy students if they are required to simulate stammering or other speech disorders in everyday, communicative situations. the stammerer usually interprets these features as predictable constituents of stammering behaviour, losing their true identity in the total subjective pattern. the interference in phonation is sustained until the stammerer is compelled to give up through lack of oxygen or until he is satisfied that sufficient effort has been expended, (c.f. van riper, 1956). some stammerers appear to depend upon a specific time-lag before releasing the word, whilst others wait until they have performed some efficacious movement or ritual. although different in form, there are basic similarities between blocking and repetitive stammering; the one involving a deliberate tonic closure whilst the other represents the employment of insufficient effort or will to overcome a corresponding resistance of the mechanism, giving rise to oscillation or "hunting" (wolf, 1959). when the stammerer is in a state of panic he perceives his own behaviour only through vague tactile and kinaesthetic impressions. visual feedback is usually not operative and auditory feedback has either broken down, or is wrongly interpreted. the feeling of relief and even success on utterance of the word serve as adequate reinforcement for the conviction that the word will not "come out" without invoking this blind intervention. his lack of awareness of what has actually occurred is manifest in his firm belief of the "difficulty" of certain sounds and words and his incredulous rejection of any suggestion that he is] interfering. ! this general pattern of behaviour is discernable in individuals widely divergent in temperament, intellectual ability, social background arid experience. there is an interesting parallel in some of the findings quoted by sargarit (1957) from his studies of indoctrination, which may give a clue to some of the factors in the development and perpetuation of stammering. "the normal extravert, for instance, seems to be 'got at' more easily, and his new patterns maintained, by quite crude and nonspecific group excitatory methods, provided that they result in a strong, continued and often repeated emotional arousal .the obsessional person or the intravert may be more unresponsive to such an approach". sargant offers ample evidence of the ways in which emotion, confusion, hunger and general debilitation facilitate the establishment of new attitudes and corresponding changes in behaviour . the study of cybernetics promises to be a further aid in our understanding of the problem. many attempts have been made to interfere with the stammerer's auditory feedback, delaying it or eliminating it with the aid of "white" sound. some of the inferences drawn from such intervention are rivalled only by those of the stammerer himself, without the advantage of expensive or ingenious equipment. the laryngeal behaviour in stammering is of paramount importance in that it involves little or no conscious perception of feedback. there appears to exist a dichotomy between motor skills learnt with the aid of visual and kinaesthetic feedback and those dependent upon auditory feedback. in view of the remarkable rapidity with which articulatory skills are acquired and applied, and the comparative ease with which they are performed by the vast majority of human beings, are there not grounds for the assumption that learning to articulate is purely a matter of organising the maturing 'endogenous' behaviour patterns to meet the requirements of one's linguistic environment? (gesell 1942, ballard and bond 1960). even some stammerers with a high degree of insight find it hard to believe that effort on their part will not facilitate speech but inhibit it, that their simple movements of lips, tongue or larynx which resemble speech sounds are in fact only caricatures of the real process. we would suggest that stammering and some of the dysphonias represent patterns of learned interference dependant upon some element of 'unconscious control'. when a stammerer realises that a situation demands communication, this realisation gives rise to an emotional reaction. it is this reaction which disrupts the hierarchies subserving homeostatic control of behaviour. in stamdecember, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) mering and a few other activities including, some have suggested, arithmetic and sport, initial failure is taken by society to be a sign that the individual is in fact incapable of improving his performance. when the demand is as frequent as it is in speech, reinforcement of negative attitudes is especially facilitated. therapy. a. change of attitude towards stammering. this is principally carried out through: (1) elimination of avoidance at every possible level. (2) re-evaluation of audience reactions and social penalties. (3) building up his resistance to these social penalties, and to communication stress, both within and without the group situation. (4) encouraging observation and discouraging theory. (5) increasing his knowledge of the psychology, physiology and phonetics of stammering, and of normal speech. (6) development of a sense of humour in studying stammering, audience reactions, and speech situations as a whole. undoubtedly, avoidance re-inforces the fear of specific sounds, words and situations, and is probably one of the most important maintaining factors in this functionally autonomous disorder. whilst avoidance continues, however subtle the avoidance may be, this is a fairly sure sign that the stammerer's attitude has not undergone sufficient change. he is obviously still embarrassed and anxious to hide the fact that he is a stammerer as far as this is possible. we are all familiar with substitutions of one word for another, but avoidance takes many forms: using the selfservice store for motives other than convenience; using ticket machines rather than approach a booking-office clerk; giving the bus conductor the correct change for your fare, to avoid stating your destination — the list is endless. the stammerer will not be prepared to abandon avoidance in its many manifestations until he is really convinced of the fact that it is largely responsible for maintaining his speech fears, and secondly, that when he begins to eliminate avoidance his speech fears begin to decrease. only so much can be accomplished by discussion: in the final analysis, he must begin to enter the situations and use the words he is tempted to avoid before he can really discover how pernicious avoidance is. this can be a difficult step, and here the well-integrated group can be of immense value. students are sent out in pairs to enter various "avoided" situations, to study their own reactions and those of the listener as objectively as possible. this results in a decrease in both the emotional attitude and in the physical concomitants of fear. one of the most important effects of such an assignment is the very illuminating reevaluation that takes place on audience reactions. we believe that it is the stammerer's assessment of "what the listener thinks" that largely determines his attitude towards his stammer, and we must recognise that imagined social penalties are just as potent in their effect as real penalties. it is just because he is anxious not "to make a fool of himself" and to invoke such penalties,that so much of his energy is devoted towards postponing, disguising and avoiding stammering. the ironic fact is, that far from achieving its purpose, much of this behaviour only adds to the overt abnormality. it is essential, therefore, if we are to discourage such avoidance, and reduce the stammerer's fear and embarrassment, that we encourage him to look more closely and objectively at how people behave when he stammers. with this in mind, he is asked to report not only on his own behaviour and feelings, but to study the behaviour of the listener. such observations can then be confirmed or denied by his partner. it frequently comes as a revelation that such a high proportion of people show so little reaction, even in the face of marked abnormality. he realises that those who do react are anxious (not unnaturally) to help him, or are as embarrassed as he is, and look away to spare them both! no attempt is made to convince the stammerer that really adverse reactions cannot occur on occasions, but he discovers from his / own experience how rare these are, and furthermore begins to appreciate that the listener probably has bigger problems than he has! through the experience itself, by relating it afterwards, and then through group discussion, he comes to recognise the psychological common denominators underlying much human behaviour. practical experience of this kind, even during a single evening, results in a marked decrease in 2 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) much of his previously subjective evaluation, and a not surprising reduction in the frequency and severity of his blocks. he comes to realise that his own emotional feelings may have caused him to make the most exaggerated interpretations of audience reactions frequently on the slimmest possible evidence of previous unpleasant experience. bizarre assignments may merely appeal to a sense of bravado, and tend to favour overcompensation, but the right type of assignment, and the good natured penalties of other members of the group, all help to build up the stammerer's emotional "resistance" to social penalties should they occur. as we have already said, however, with increasing experience of every kind of feared and previously avoided situation, he grows steadily more objective and less emotional in his approach to the people to whom he must speak. humour, which tends to arise quite spontaneously when these outside activities are related, is undoubtedly a tremendously potent factor in producing a change in attitude. we never have to ask a stammerer to tell a funny story about stammering — almost every assignment they carry out seems to be potentially humourous, and they waste no time in making the most of the story! our libyan student has his own favourite assignment: first he asks an unsuspecting member of the public what they think about stammerers. having removed the ground from under their feet with this question, he then proceeds to ask what advice they would give to help a stammerer. the final coup de grace is to pose the following question: "if you had a sister, would you be prepared to give her hand in marriage to a stammerer?". our favourite response: "well, of course. after all, he can't help it, can he?" direct observations and experience discourage the kind of theorising which tend to handicap ithe stammerer in his thinking, and which seldom have little to add to the knowledge of either student or therapist. we give a broad joutline of both past and current therapy, {so that he may consider them against the background of his own practical experience in the group, and in carrying out assignments. through this, and the study of stammering behaviour about to be described, we set out to dispel the myth and mystery surrounding stammering, and to encourage as far as we possibly can an enlightened and objective co-operation from the members of the group. b. modification of stammering behaviour. when a member of the group has reached a stage at which he has been able to give up some measure of avoidance, and most of the delaying techniques he has previously employed, he is then encouraged to adopt his new, experimental approach towards modification of his stammering behaviour. within this group, using communicative situations (e.g. answering questions, interviewing, giving short talks, reading aloud and play reading) he must first endeavour to free himself of both avoidance and interjection. the deterioration in fluency which may result calls for immediate reassurance and support. senior members of the group prove particularly helpful at this stage. when stammering occurs, he is encouraged to consider whether or not it was his own interference which prevented the utterance of the word. it is suggested that he rejects the idea of forcing out the word. when he stammers, he normally feels he has little or no control over the situation. with the encouragement and friendly militance of the group, he will still stammer, but his total emotional reaction becomes markedly reduced. he is better able to take note of what is happening, and can begin to deal with the problem. communicative demand is built up in such a way that when blocking occurs it is often greeted by laughter from everyone including the speaker. his gradual refusal to "conform" to the usual rules, requirements and rituals of the stammering, and his adoption of a more phlegmatic approach, are strongly reminiscent of sargant's principles of resistance to "brain-washing". it is our belief that the emotional reactions of fear, and of aggression, must be extracted from the everyday speech situations, to allow a more mature approach. this is also achieved by the open discussion of the feelings which surround stammering. the therapist must alternate between a directive and non-directive role, taking advantage of the friendly rivalry soon developed within the group. it is important to note that the group is rarely aggressive towards any one member. we are, in fact, continually impressed by the tolerance shown towards new members and towards those december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) few who verge on the psychoneurotic, exhibiting a degree of acceptance rarely found in many other social groups. understanding of the mechanics of stammering is fostered by straightforward descriptions of the anatomy and physiology of the systems involved, together with an outline of basic phonetic principles. practical application of these principles is achieved by use of a mirror and throat microphones (ex. r.a.f. and u.s.a.a.f. originally suggested by an r.a.f. wireless fitter attending one of our hospital groups). we were interested to learn of similar work being carried out by breckwoldt (1962) in which his "laryngoreflectoscope" can be used to show stammerers their laryngeal activity. he observes that gutzman mentioned the value of autolaryngoscopy in his "sprachheilkunde". these "feed-back" techniques help the stammerer to achieve control of his behaviour through increased understanding. he must then begin to employ his new approach to blocking in "field work". the change in behaviour requires constant stabilisation for at least a period of several months. those who attend irregularly through choice or circumstance tend to take the longest period. we have found, however, that there is rarely a relapse where a major alteration in behaviour and attitude occurs. conclusion. therapeutically, class organisation is unique: although we are allowed to limit our regular attendance to 7 or 8 students in a group, should attendance drop below this, then classes can automatically be closed. in this respect, we enjoy no special privileges. far from being a disadvantage, we realise that this has proved to be an excellent discipline and a stimulating challenge. it has encouraged us, of sheer necessity, to evolve an approach which is designed to integrate a group as rapidly as possible; to create an atmosphere of informality which the students enjoy; and finally, a therapy designed to produce reasonably rapid results. as therapy has been streamlined and developed in this direction, two trends have been noticeable: that few stammerers now cease attendance without reason or explanation, and secondly, a much higher proportion reenrol for the second or third year which is necessary in many cases. our therapy has evolved from experience and observation of many stammerers, just as the stammerer in the group must finally learn from his own observations and experiences. atmosphere in the group is hard to describe. as we have stressed, informality and humour are an essential part. it seems to have been evolved in much the same way as the therapy. these are a few of the factors which have helped to determine it: in the first place, these are students in a study group, not patients in a clinic: they are treated as people with a common problem, not patients with a defect; secondly, they are free to come and discuss particular difficulties before or after class, and individual help is made available whenever necessary; thirdly, every member is given an equal opportunity to participate and succeed (and it is surprising how many students volunteer to participate on their first evening, and find, perhaps for the first time, that they are laughing about their own stammer as well as everyone else's); finally, no attempt is made to create an "easy" atmosphere to facilitate fluency — on the contrary — but the tolerance and humour of the group reduce any emotional trauma to a level which the student can handle, however new or embarrassed he may be. for the past three years, students have completed a comprehensive questionnaire within a few weeks of joining the group. this helped us to know much more, in the absence of initial interviews, about the stammerer and his attitudes, environment, personality and problems. this year, however, it was decided that some way must be found of measuring progress as objectively as possible so that we can systematically assess the results achieved by this particular approach. measurement of progress in stammering has always presented special problems. we hope that the form which has been prepared will go a considerable way in overcoming the difficulties of objective evaluation. the form is simple and complete. it will be given to the stammerer four times: at the commencement of the session, and then at the end of the first, second and third terms. it is divided into four parts: the therapist's check list for symptoms of stammering; the stammerer's check list of situations he avoids; the stammerer's own check list of 2 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) stammering behaviour (e.g. looking away from the listener, taking a breath before a difficult word); finally, his check list which indicates his assessment of audience reactions: (put a tick against the things people do when you stammer: look away laugh look embarrassed or uncomfortable try to help you out with a word appear impatient behave sympathetically treat you as if you are stupid appear superior or condescending generally react unfavourably show little or no reaction.) a decrease in the number of items ticked will be an indication of both changes in attitudes and of decrease in severity and abnormality of speech behaviour. summary. the london county council has for many years held evening classes for adult stamr merers. since 1956, when one of the authors was appointed as therapist, general relaxation as a fundamental form of treatment was abandoned in favour of "non-avoidance" techniques. groups consist of stammerers ranging in age from 16 to 55 years. in addition to this wide age range, there are varied racial, social and educational backgrounds. 8 is considered to be the "ideal" number of students for a therapeutic group. the approach to therapy has been based on the assumption that the stammerer is a "normal" person with a normal speech mechanism, and that stammering can be considered as a functionally autonomous disorder of communication. basically the approach to the problem is two-fold: ;the first and most important is to change the attitude towards the stammer; the second is:to bring about change and modifications in the abnormal speech behaviour. therapies which do not attempt to effect a change in attitude towards stammering, but are directed towards achieving speech fluency, are bound to have limited or transient effect. opsomming. die londonse distriksraad het vir baie jare aandklasse gereel vir volwasse hakkelaars. sedert 1956, toe een van die skrywers as 'n terapeut aangestel is, is algemene verslapping as 'n grondbeginsel in terapie afgeskaf tengunste van die „nie-ontwykings" tegniek. die groepe bestaan uit hakkelaars, wat wissel in ouderdom van 16 tot 55 jaar. behalwe vir die groot verskil in ouderdomme, het die groep ook persone ingesluit met 'n verskillende rasse-, sosiale en opvoedkundige agtergrond. agt studente word beskou as die ideale aantal per groep. die benadering tot terapie is gebaseer op die veronderstelling dat die hakkelaar 'n ,.normale" persoon is met 'n normale spraakmeganisme, en dat hakkel as 'n funksioneeloutonomiese gebrek van kommunikasie, beskou kan word. die benadering tot die probleem is basies tweeledig: die eerste en belangrikste is om die houding teenoor die hakkel te verander; die twede is om veranderings en wysigings in die abnormale spraakpatroon teweeg te bring. behandelings wat nie mik om die houding teenoor die hakkel te verander nie, maar wat gesteld is om vlotheid van spraak te verkry, moet noodwendig beperkte en verbygaande resultate lewer. references 1. ballard. c. f. .& bond, ε. k„ "clinical observations on the correlation between variations of jaw form and variations of oro-facial behaviour, including those for articulation". speech pathology & therapy, 2, 55-63. (1960). 2. breckwoldt, g. h., "the laryngo-reflectoscope " (unpublished m.s.s.) (1957). 3. foulkes, s. h. & anthony, e. j., "group psychotherapy". london: penguin books. 4. gesell, α., "morphologies of the mouth and mouth behaviour". american journal of orthodontics, 27, 397. (1942). 5. johnson, w., "perceptual and evaluational factors in stuttering". handbook of speech pathology, ed. travis, l. e. new york: appleton century crofts. (1957). 6. kenyon, e. l„ "the etiology of stammering: the psycho-physiological facts which concern the production of speech sounds and stammering". journal of speech disorders, 8, 337-348. (1943). 7. sargant, w., "battle for the mind — a physiology of conversion and brain washing". london; wm. heinemann. (1957). 8. van riper, c., "speech correction". new york: prentice hall. inc. (1956). 9. williams, d. e., "masseter muscle action potentials in stuttered and non-stuttered speech". journal of speech and hearing disorders, 20, 242-261. (1955). 10. williams, d. e., "a point of view about 'stuttering' " journal of speech and hearing disorders, 22, 390-397. (1957). 11. wolf, a. a. & wolf, e. g., "feedback processes in the theory of certain speech disorders". speech pathology and therapy, 2, 48-55. (1959). december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 3 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an application of learning theory to the secondary aspects of stuttering m a r g a r e t m a r k s , b.a. (log), m.a. (witwatersrand) senior lecturer, department of speech pathology & audiology, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg summary t h e c o n c e p t s of primary and secondary stuttering are re-evoked t o provide a framework for a discussion of the theoretical division of stuttering into a series of responses. t h e stuttering behaviours of an advanced stutterer are divided into those of awareness, avoidance, t h e ' m o m e n t of stuttering', a release response and the utterance of the word. one set of responses, t h o s e of avoidance, are treated w i t h i n various learning t h e o r y paradigms, n a m e l y , t h e mowrer-ullman h y p o t h e s i s , chaining, superstition, and an avoidance-escape d i c h o t o m y . a l t h o u g h n o direct therapeutic implications are d e d u c e d from these analogies, the value for the speech pathologist of a k n o w l e d g e of the principles and t e c h n i q u e s of learning t h e o r y , is stressed. opsomming die k o n s e p t e van primere en sekondere hakkel w o r d weer eens bygebring o m 'n raamwerk te verskaf vir 'n bespreking van die teoretiese verdeling van hakkel in 'n reeks response. die hakkelgedrag van 'n gevorderde hakkelaar w o r d ingedeel in b e w u s t h e i d , vermyding, die „ o o m b l i k van hakkel", 'n vrylatingsrespons en die uiting van die w o o r d . een stel response, vermyding, w o r d b e s k o u uit verskillende leerteoretiese paradigmas, naamlik die mowrer-ullman hipotese, aaneenskakeling, b y g e l o o f en 'n vermydings-ontvlugtingsdigotomie. a l h o e w e l geen direkte terapeutiese implikasies afgelei word van hierdie analogies nie, word die waarde van b e k e n d h e i d m e t hierdie beginsels en tegnieke van die leerteorie vir die spraakheelkundige b e k l e m t o o n . although the terms primary and secondary stuttering have fallen into disrepute, and for good semantic reasons, it seems that a case could be ma'de to conjure them up again, to differentiate between the 'actual' stutter (block, dysfluency, classically conditioned response); and those behaviours which the individual uses in an attempt to cope with this undesirable stutter. while the writer is cognizant of the fact that these two aspects of the stutter are not necessarily mutually exclusive, and that the behaviours described as primary and secondary can co-exist and be displayed by the same stutterer at different times and indeed even in the same stuttering incident, it is the aim of this article to focus on the secondary aspects of the responses which constitute the efforts of the more advanced stutterer to deal with his dysfluency. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20 december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an application of learning theory to stuttering 69 there are many theories which attempt to explain the original, or primary speech breakdown, but there is little serious opposition to the theory which best explains the acquisition of secondary responses, namely, that theory derived from the laws of learning. van riper1 1 does not feel that the onset of stuttering can be explained only by learning, but he states t h a t . . we are convinced that much of the behavior which goes under the name of stuttering is learned behavior. we can account for its variability of symptoms and its consistency of pattern in no other way. we have watched it being learned.'2 the only other theoreticians who attempt to explain the secondary responses are those who are psychodynamically oriented and, although many speech pathologists would reject such descriptions of secondary symptoms as being revealing of'motor patterns of nursing . . . . cannibalistic muscle patterns, analretentive and anal-expulsive' (coriat, as quoted by glauber3), more acceptance is shown of the principles behind such phrases as 'aggressive symptoms' and 'secondary gains', and some of the intractability of severe stuttering is often ascribed to the stutterer's 'need' to retain his stutter, rather than to the therapist's need to know more about the alleviation of his patient's abnormal speech behaviour. neurophysiology is a field which could provide a rationale for some of the bizarre responses which comprise advanced stuttering; within this framework we could possibly seek an explanation for the distractive devices which remain, like albatrosses round the stutterer's neck, in the form of secondary behaviours. these, and other phenomena noted in stuttering, may become logical when considered in neurophysiological terms. generally, however, as it has come to be accepted that most, if not all, the secondary responses are learned, the understanding of their acquisition has subsumed a knowledge of the laws of learning, while the therapeutic implications have been based on the principles of behaviour modification. once the worker is committed to this theoretical viewpoint, he stands to benefit from the findings and knowledge of an extensive literature; he is also faced with the responsibilities which such a commitment, and such a literature, imply. the writer has, in an earlier issue of this journal6, discussed these responsibilities, and considered the difficulties which can arise from a limited understanding, and consequent application, of learning theory as it pertains to stuttering. for many speech pathologists not formally trained in learning theory, it seems a far cry from the puzzled rat that does not know which way to turn, and from the eccentricities of pigeons' pecking (and of the people who pick out what pigeons should peck at), to the human creature struggling to talk. however, we must realise that a simplistic view of the relationship between animal and human behaviour no longer exists. most contemporary learning theorists have accepted the complexity of animal behaviour, and are even more aware of the almost unchartable vastness of complicated human behaviour. they are aware, for example, that cognition and language must be considered when accounting for many aspects of human functioning. terrace,9 when defining a particular operation of the learning process (stimulus control) states that 'many topics such as perception, psychophysics, thinking and psycholinguist i c s . . . are directly suggested by this definition, and (there are) numerous other topics that are not so directly relevant.' tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 margaret marks as previously stated, it is probably in a description of the secondary, acquired behaviours, the speaker's attempts to do something about the feared word or situation, that concepts culled from learning theory are most directly applicable. some speech pathologists have attempted to use procedures, notably those of reinforcement, to demonstrate control of stuttering. the disparity of results, particularly those of experimenters using punishment, lead brutten and shoemaker1 to their two-factor theory, a concept which supports the present writer's view, stated in 19665, that previous workers had thought of 'the stutter' in too molar, and not molecular enough, a manner and that, in most stuttering incidents shown by an advanced stutterer, there is a.sequence of responses which comprise the motor act of stuttering. in this paper, an attempt is made to represent, diagrammatically, this series of responses and to discuss how learning theory can be used to explain one set of these responses, i.e. the avoidance responses. the following responses are suggested as being relatively typical of an incident of stuttering; although not all the behaviours occur each time the person stutters, they appear with enough frequency to warrant inclusion: is the discriminatory stimulus the awareness of approaching difficulty are the avoidance responses is the ' m o m e n t of stuttering' — primary stuttering is the release response is utterance of the w o r d figure 1: a stuttering incident comprising a series of responses. a brief description of each of these sets of responses is given. / a awareness. something in the stimulus configuration of the speaker's environment acts as a discriminatory stimulus, which initiates the series of responses. β avoidances. these are the responses which have been called by therapists, starters, postponements, interjections, etc. for;this paper they will be described only as avoidances. they will be discussed in more depth later, but a graphic representation here indicates their nature. if 'town' is the feared word in the sentence 'i'm going to town', the following diagrams represent two types of avoidance responses: journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an application of learning theory to stuttering 71 (i) i'm going to., going to., t-t-town β ε (ii) i'm going to the city α β / ν ε i'm going to figure 2: representations of avoidances. in (i), the avoidances were not effective as, in addition to using them, the person experienced the stutter c, before uttering the word and so achieving his goal, that of communication. in (ii), the avoidance, this time one of circumlocution, was successful, and he did not experience c, the moment of stuttering, and its consequent d, the release response. although the desired word was not said, the goal of communication was reached. c the moment of stuttering, is represented by various behaviours; the order in which they are listed below indicates increasing amounts of tension. ο it is possible that there is, in some incidents of stuttering, no 'moment of stuttering', i.e. the stutterer may experience awareness and may utilise responses described under β but, even if he had not done so, would have experienced none of the behaviours described immediately below. r v / v ^ repetition, either easy or tense, but not as tense or rapid as the behaviours described as tremors. this stage could, depending on the phoneme stuttered on, be represented thus , i.e. as a prolongation. neither of these (repetition and prolongation) is actually a dysfluency, in that they do not interfere with the rhythm of speech, or cause a break in the flow of continued speech. block, the actual cessation of ongoing speech movement; it seems as if this occurs mainly, if not only, on stops and vowels. i tremor, 'fast little vibrations... produced by highly tensing the i muscles that form a fixed posture'. (van riper 1 0 ) tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 margaret marks clinical evidence has lead to the impression that it is the attempt, on the part of the speaker to do something about this moment of stuttering, which gives rise to so much aberrant behaviour. exactly what aspect of this response is so fearful, is a matter of conjecture; it may be, as van riper1 0 implies, the feeling of impotence when the stutterer gets thrown into a tremor; it is also possible the present writer believes, that it is the sound of repetitive speech, behaviour which was considered as 'stuttering' in childhood, and therefore taboo. parents often bring their children back to a therapist, saying, in effect: 'we're so pleased; he's stopped stuttering. of course, he does stop and take a deep breath, but he's not stuttering.' advanced stutterers, with visually bizarre responses, would rather use these, if they bring with them a reduction in the sound of 'stuttered' speech they do not see what they look like, but can hear that the feared stutter is not vocalised. 'you'll never get me to speaspeaspeak like that; it sounds ridiculous' is the gist of statements of many stutterers with severe blocks, often accompanied by very visible struggle responses. it may be that the feared aspect of the stuttering moment is not the same for each stutterer, or, indeed, may vary from one stuttering incident to the next, in the same speaker. d a release response, which terminates the moment of stuttering. an effortful release is usually more necessary when the moment of stuttering has comprised a block or tremor, than when the speaker is repeating or prolonging, as, in the latter two cases there is little or nothing to be released from. this is seen as a release, rather than an avoidance response, thus supporting van riper's11 contention that 'it is both psychologically and semantically unwise to stretch the term avoidance to include these escape reactions'. ε utterance of the word. this continuation of communication is the reinforcement of all that has gone before. once a framework such as the one above is tentatively established as a theoretical point of view, certain questions can be asked, and certain paradigms from learning theory evoked as possible explanations, or as areas deserving further study. each of the five aspects described could be rephrased in terms of some aspect of learning theory, but only avoidance behaviours and some of their learning theory correlates will be dealt with in this paper. when the patient comes to therapy, he has his set of premiums: negative for the moment of stuttering, positive for avoidance behaviours. many therapies are based on the principle of trying to get him to change these premiums, i.e. to want to get to the moment of stuttering; and to want to stop the avoidance behaviour. however, it has been the experience of many therapists that, even though the stutterer tries to stop avoiding, the 'habit' dies hard. learning theory offers various explanations for behaviours which are deleterious to the well-being of the individual and which are, nevertheless, maintained, behaviours described by wolpe12 as neurotic: 'any persistent habit of unadaptive behaviour acquired by learning in a physiologically normal organism'. theories based on the principle of learning must account for their persistence in the face of their lack of efficacy in coping in the best possible way with the. noxious situation. journal of the south african speeh and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) an application of learning theory to stuttering 73 brutten and shoemaker1 state that both wischner and sheehan use the mowrer-ullman hypothesis to explain this persistence: 'according to mowrer and ullman, if all things are equal, a response will be reinforced or extinguished depending on whether the rewarding or punishing consequences, respectively, follow the cessation of behaviour more closely in time.' thus the concept of time is considered a crucial factor in the acquisition and retention of aberrant responses. another concept which bears scrutiny is that of chaining, first described by skinner in 1938,8 and later defined by ferster and perrott2 as the phenomenon which takes place when a stimulus maintains the preceding performance and also makes possible the next performance in the chain. if, for example, within the behaviours described as β above, there is a sequence of b ( 1 ) leading to b < 2 ) leading to b < 3 ) which eventually leads, albeit painfully, to reinforcement e, utterance of the word, each response in the chain is necessary, as it makes ε possible. an early work of luper's4 although not explicitly using the term 'chaining' seems to imply the concept, as does a recent experiment reported by prins and lohr7. data from experiments on chaining must lead us to speculate on certain topics, e.g. how certain elements of the chain are learned i.e. is it possible that they are differentially learned, and will need, therefore, different types of extinguishing procedures? why are some elements in the chain (which could be called the 'core' of stuttering) present in all incidents of stuttering, while others occur infrequently? why are some 'distractions' maintained as part of the chain, while others are extinguished? how can information about extinguishing chain behaviour aid in stuttering therapy? superstitious behaviour is another description which aids in the understanding of seemingly irrational behaviour. this describes elements in the response not directly instrumental in gaining reinforcement, but which, through association with the operant behaviour, become part of the response gestalt. the classic examples are those of skinner's pigeons who emit certain responses immediately before the ones which are effective in gaining the desired goal (e.g. turning around before pecking the switch which releases the food, where turning around becomes a superstitious response). although they were coincidental to the desired behaviour, the responses become part of the total response pattern. it does not seem to be stretching analogies too far if we describe at least some of the stutterer's avoidance responses as superstitious, or ritualistic behaviour. when he considers the intransigence of some of these avoidance behaviours, the clinician will recognise their similarity to superstitious responses, traditionally difficult to eliminate. as with chaining, we must attempt to utilise knowledge gained by the learning psychologist, in an attempt to break this superstitious behaviour. animal experimentation has shown that there is a difference between the learning processes involved in avoidance and escape. avoidance behaviour is learned by the rat who is faced with an electric grid between it and its desired goal; acquisition of this type of behaviour is different to the learning of behaviours which a second rat must acquire when it is already on the grid and has to discover how to get off — escape behaviour. it seems important to differentiate between these two sets of responses in the stutterer (behaviours β and d), as it is probable that they are learned differently, require different tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 margaret marks methods of extinction, and that one set may be more difficult to extinguish than the other. no direct therapeutic implications have been drawn from the analogous nature of stuttering behaviour and that observed by experimental psychologists in their work with controlled units of behaviour, but it seems that a consideration of the relationship between certain aspects of learning and of stuttering could be of benefit to those who seek to help stutterers. there is a rich field of knowledge gained from experiments based on learning theory, and it seems desirable that speech pathologists and therapists adopt and adapt principles and techniques derived from this valuable source of data. references 1. brutten, e.j. and shoemaker, d.j. (1967): the modification of stuttering. prentice-hall inc., new jersey. 2. ferster, c.b. and perrott, m.c. (1968): behavior principles. appletoncentury-crofts, new york. 3. glauber, i.p. (1968): the psychoanalysis of stuttering. in eisenson, j. (ed.), stuttering: a symposium. harper and bros, new york. 4. luper, h.l. (1956): consistency of stuttering in relation to the goal gradient hypothesis./, of speech and hearing dis., 21, 336-342. 5. marks, m. (1966): stuttering viewed as a sequence of responses. paper presented to international seminar in stuttering and behavior therapy. published in gray, b.b. and england, g. (1969) stuttering and the conditioning therapies. monterey institute for speech and hearing, california. 6. marks, m. (1968): are we good behaviourists? /. of the south african logppedic society, 15,19-25. 7. prins, d. and lohr, f. (1972): behavioral dimensions of stuttered speech. /. of speech and hearingres. 15, 61-71. 8. skinner, b.f. (1938): the behavior of organisms. appleton-centurycrofts, new york. 9. terrace, h.s. (1966): stimulus control. in honig, w.k. (ed.) operant behavior: areas of research and application appleton-century-crofts, new york. 10. van riper, c. (1963): speech correction, principles and methods. (4th edition) prentice hall inc., new jersey. 11. van riper, c. (1971): the nature of stuttering. prentice-hall inc., new jersey. 12. wolpe, j. (1958): psychotherapy by reciprocal inhibition. stanford university press, stanford and witwatersrand university press, johannesburg. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) philips hearing aid services a division of s.a. philips (pty) ltd. hearing aids portable audiometers group teaching systems philips hearing aid services head office 1005 cavendish chambers, 183 jeppe street, p.o. box 3069, johannesburg. p h i l i p s @)44923 tydskrifvan die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) experiments on the elasticity of the vocal cords d a m s t e , p . h . , m.d. a n d wieneke, g.h., ph.d. af deling foniatrie, universiteits kno-kliniek, catharijnesingel 101, utrecht, nederland summary regulation of vocal p i t c h occurs b y changes in the length and t e n s i o n o f the vocal folds. because the young's m o d u l u s of t h e vocal cord ligament c o u l d b e a factor w h i c h determines the difference b e t w e e n male and female voices, this was made an object o f research. n o differences could b e s h o w n . s o m e of the problems and t h e limited accuracy of t h e s e experiments are reviewed. opsomming beheer van s t e m t o o n h o o g t e vind plaas deur veranderinge in the lengte en spanning van die s t e m v o u e . aangesien y o u n g se m o d u l u s van die stemlipligam e n t 'n faktor kan w e e s wat die verskil tussen manlike en vroulike s t e m m e bepaal, was dit die o n d e r w e r p van navorsing. geen verskille w o r d a a n g e t o o n nie. s o m m i g e van die p r o b l e m e en die beperkte akkuraatheid van hierdie eksperimente w o r d bespreek. when the pitch of the voice rises, the length of the vocal cords increases. all specialists in voice agree with this, no matter how they differ in other opinions. the mechanism by which the length of the vocal cords increases is well known; the backward turning of the cricoid cartilage in respect to the thyroid ,by the pull of the cricothyroids increases the distance from the arytenoids to the farthest attachment point of the vocal cords. the amount of stretch of the vocal folds which can be effected by this physiological mechanism was specified by sonninen12 with the help of x-rays. from this research came a paper on the lengthening mechanism, more accurate than that given by r. schilling10 in 1940. van den berg5 proposed by means of experiments with cadaver larynges, that the vocal cords can stretch to a maximum of 30% of their original length. in addition, the tension (in other words the elasticity modulus) increases approximately proportional to the traction exercised on the tissue. falsetto tones are produced by the fully stretched position of the vocal ligament and conus elasticus. these tones are at one end of the vocal, register scale, and at the other end are the tones of the chest register, the normal speaking voice of man. this type of voice is generated by the completely relaxed and supple vocal folds, in which all parts participate,in the vibration. for a melodious speaking voice, journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20 december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e x p e r i m e n t s on the elasticity of the vocal cords 15 , f u r s e above all for singing, it is important that higher tones are proh d in a mixed or a middle register. in this case the tensing of the ligament h t h e cricothyroid is combined with the tensing and shortening of the muscles h c o n t r a c t i o n of the internal and external thyroarytenoids. thus the vibraγ of the ligament remains coupled with that of the muscle mass, which gives e to the full tone of the normal register. in falsetto register the vibration is γ ot t r a n s m i t t e d to the whole vocal fold, only the medial edge vibrates. this "suits in a weak, thin tone. the voice can be voluntarily controlled to a great extent, and any individual can make a large variety of vocal sounds. f r o m just the sound of the voice it is possible to form an idea of the person, his character and his mood. one has, as it were, an acoustic face, from the sound of the voice. damage to the voice is just as disturbing as a facial scar. a woman with a voice which has become masculine through the use of androgens, has suffered an irreparable mutilation. her acoustic impression has lost a great deal of prettiness. she can shave or wax away her facial hair, but her cracking baritone cannot be hidden, except by not going out or by keeping quiet. the problem of virilisation of the voice became noticed in the years after 1960 through the introduction of anabolic s t e r o i d s . 1 ' 2 ' 6 , 9 it is not possible to guess how many women have been made victims by the unchecked use of these drugs. it is still a real problem. when one has seen it once, it is apparent that it is not rare, particularly the early stages of voice virilisation. in the early stages the symptoms are not so obvious. characteristic symptoms are: unsteady vocal pitch which wavers between two timbres; a light and darkone. it is the lack of control which gives the voice a subjective strange sensation. the range is not decreased, rather the lower range increased, and the higher tones less well controlled than before. this is only noticed by women who sing. vocal cord oedema comes especially into consideration for differential diagnosis; the pitch range in the higher tones is severely limited through this. however this is not the case in the early stages of virilisation. mirror examination shows a great difference. in vocal cord oedema there is a widening of the vocal folds, which are lighter coloured. laryngeal stroboscopy shows greater vibration amplitude and a widening of the wavecrest. there is frequently nothing special to see in virilised vocal cords. only in a later stage is there sometimes a change in colour, to a dark yellow. what change in the vocal folds is responsible for the change of voice in the woman whose voice becomes masculine? that is the question which many have put, and which has many different answers. some have sought the cause outside the larynx, in timing of conduction of impulses in the nervous system. we can forget this possibility, as it is based on a most improbable theory of vocal cord vibration. serious thought has been given to the opinion that the change is due to a change in muscle-tissue in the vocal folds, the enlargment being directly caused by the anabolic* effect of testerone and other steroid compounds. another opinion is that change in the mucous membrane causes the change in voice timbre. perello9 has studied a section from a mutated * by this is meant the property to promote the synthesis of muscle protein. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 16 damste and wieneke larynx and reports a thickening of the middle layer of epithelium. in the same article he gives the increase in volume and power of the thyroarytenoids and the inspiration muscles as causes of the deepening of the voice. in differential diagnosis of vocal cord oedema, it is possible to find arguments which make the thickening of the epithelium as one of the causes of a voice change less probable. it is also known that steroids, to which the androgen hormone belongs, have an effect on the composition of connective tissue. since the connective tissue in the vocal folds (coni elastici and vocal cord ligaments) greatly determines the sort of voice and its quality, the hypothesis is that changes in this shall account for masculine voice in women. some physio-chemical arguments have been discussed by damste.7 experiments in 1963-1964, we studied the possibility of whether the elasticity of the connective tissue in the edge of the vocal cords, could be determined by measuring the elasticity. experiments were carried out on a score of larynges. the results were in many cases not completely reliable due to complicating circumstances and that is the reason why the work has not been published until now. on closer examination of the data we feel that it is still useful to publish the method used, and a few of the results, as a guide and a warning to potential researchers. the plan was to determine the young's modulus of a satisfactorily large number of vocal cords from men, women, women with masculine voices and possibly children and to see if the values of the young's modulus in the four groups were significantly different. method the elongation of the vocal cord and the force was measured with the apparatus shown in figure 1. the ends of the specimen were clamped. one end was fixed to the support and one to the arm of a lever made from a light and stiff hollow pipe. on the other side of the axis of rotation the end of a spring was attached. the spring could be stretched by displacing the other end by a nylon thread which is wound round an axis behind the black knob (below right in figure 1). the force exerted by the spring is indicated in grams on a gauged scale. the long arm of the balance indicates the elongation of the specimen in tenths of a millimeter. procedure an experiment with forces from 0-60 grams in 5 grm intervals was done within a minute, the time necessary to write or to dictate the numbers. more time was necessary for the preparation of the specimens. to get a solid attachment of the ligament a piece from the thyroid cartilage and from the arytenoid was taken with it. the measurement was done after the specimens had lain in a physiological salt solution for ten minutes. measurements with more or less dried out specimens were not reliable. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e x p e r i m e n t s on the elasticity of the vocal cords 17 figure 1. the apparatus with which the stress-strain curves of the vocal cord were measured. the vocal cord was fixed on the left side with two clamps. in the middle is the scale for force, to the right the scale for increase in length. see text (methods) for further explanation. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 18 damste and wieneke results figure 2. tension 6 as a function of the elasticity ε of the right vocal cord from a woman who died at 40 years. an example of the results is shown in figuie 2. the stress (force/area of the cross-section) is given against the strain (increase in length/length with 0 gram force). the cross-section of the vocal cord was calculated from the weight with the assumptions that (a) the area of the cross-section is equal over the whole length (b) the mass density of the tissue is 1 grm.cm-3. in these preliminary measurements the results of 8 male vocal cords and 10 female were considered. in all cases the stiffness (young's modulus measured as a slope on the curve: δ6/δε) increased greatly with increasing strain (to 15 *). the value journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e x p e r i m e n t s on the elasticity of the vocal cords 19 f the strain, above which the curves rise steeply, shows noticeably great differences. these differences were clearly correlated with the age of the patient on death. the relation between the age and the strain where the young's modulus is one half of the maximum value measured in the experiment is shown in figure 3. also, the young's modulus at the beginning of the 8 • • ο ο η ο 8 _l 0 25 50 75 100 [ y e a r ] age • figure 3. value of the elasticity ε 5 at which the young's modulus of the vocal cord is equal to half the maximum measured value set out against the age of the person at death. shaded blocks = male, open circles = female, correlation co-efficient = 0.8. . curve (0-5 grf) shows the dependance on age. for example the young's modulus measured from a girl of 6 years was 350 grf.cm-2 and from a woman of 40 years 550 grf.cm-2. the maximum error of the apparatus is about 20% in this range. in the normal voice range the elasticity at small stresses will be important. the young's modulus in the range of strain less than 25% does not show significant differences in vocal cords from males and females without masculine voice change. this is partly due to the lack of precision of the measurements but mainly to the influence of the age of the person. conclusions from these measurements no significant difference between the young's modulus for low stress values in males or females could be determined. consequently no conclusion can be drawn as to how far masculine changes of the female voice are a result of change in the young's modulus. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 20 damste and wieneke possibly age has a greater influence on the vocal cords than use of drugs. we must first have a better insight into the changes in the vocal apparatus due to age, before it can be concluded that the changes in voice with the use of virilising drugs are due to changes in elasticity. it seems advisable to investigate other factors which have an effect on these changes. one can think of the increase in the mass of the intrinsic laryngeal muscles. the accuracy of the elasticity modulus is one of the obstructions to interpret low strain (<25%) values. in view of the discovered differences the maximum error had to be smaller than 40 grf.cm-2 (7%). to achieve this, the measuring apparatus had to meet the following requirements: the zero length must be measured with an accuracy of ±0.2 mm; the elongation with ± 0.02 mm; the force with ± 0.05 grf; the weight with ±0.5 mgr. maximum error. this includes the uncertainties with regard to the true length of the vocal cord, the varying cross-section and deviations in mass density. in the statistic analysis the effect of age has to be taken into account. discussion the conclusions do not confirm our hypothesis that the young's modulus in men and women with virilised voices have lower values than in normal women. therefore they are not in agreement with the results of bauer4 which did confirm this hypothesis. he described a clear relationship between the young's modulus and the sex (ratio 2:1) and could also discern the effects of voice mutating substances. bauer calculates the values for the young's modulus at a 50 grm weight. according to our measurements the tissue is then already in the range of a high young's modulus, at which it probably does not reach during normal voice use. the value of the young's modulus according to bauer, is somewhat smaller than that found by us: 600-1300 grf.cm-2 against 1800-4500 grf.cm-2 measured with a stress of 50 grm. references 1. arndt, h.j. (1963): stimmschaden bei frauen durch androgene und anabole hormone. deutsche med. woch. 88: 2336-2339. 2. bauer, h. (1963): die beeinflussung der weiblichen stimme durch androgene hormone.folia phoniatrica 15: 264-268. 3. bauer, h. und konig, w. (1967): physikalische und histologische untersuchungen am normalen und einem weiblichen virilisierten kehlkopf. arch. ohr. nas.-kehlk. heilk. 188: 358-363. 4. bauer, h. (1968): die beziehungen der phoniatrie zur endokrinologie. folia phoniatrica 20: 387-393. 5. berg van den, jw. (1960): current probl. phoniat. logop., karger, baselnew york 19-34. 6. damste, p.h. (1964): virilisation of the voice due to anabolic steroids. folia phoniatrica 16:10-18. 7. damste, p.h. (1964): virile changes in the voice induced by androgens. excerpta medica intern. congress 85: 158-160. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 20, december 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) e x p e r i m e n t s on the elasticity of the vocal cords 21 9. 10. 11. 12. ί έ ρ η (1967): voice change in adult women caused by virilizing agents journ. of speech and hearing disorders, 32: 126-132. perello j. (1964): virilization de la laringe femenina.^cta o.r.l. iber.amer xv, 2: 139-141. schilling, r. (1940): uber den spannungsmechanismus der stimmlippen hals-nasenund ohrenarzt 31: 1γ2. sonninen, a. (1956): the role of the external laryngeal muscles in length adjustment of the vocal cords in singing. acta otolaryngol suppl. 130, stockholm. sonninen, a. (1962): paratasis-gram of the vocal folds and the dimensions of the voice. proc. 4th intern. congr. phonetics (helsinki), ed. mouton, den haag 250-258. for all y o u r m e d i c a l b o o k a n d j o u r n a l r e q u i r e m e n t s consult t h e s p e c i a l i s t s p . b . m a y e r 902 n o r w i c h house heerengracht c a p e t o w n telephone 2-9231 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheelkunde, vol. 20, desember 1973 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society the treatment of the cleft lip and palate patient a. jacobson, b.d.s. (rand), m.s. (orth.) (chicago) and c. j. dreyer, b.sc.(s.a.), b.d.s., h.d.d. (rand.), department of dentistry, university of the witwatersrand. introduction children with cleft lips and/or palates are cripples in the truest sense of the word. the physical appearance of these children is unpleasant, mastication and deglutition is upset, and the speech is often unintelligible. as a result of these aberrations the child is likely to become a psychological misfit, yet these children, other than their physical defect, are normal and should become wellintegrated and useful adult citizens. the treatment of these individuals is usually of such long duration and complexity that a co-ordinated treatment plan should be formulated for them by a cleft-palate unit, and not by each individual who happens to be treating such a patient at the time. the member of a cleft-palate unit will, by virtue of discussion, be able to appreciate all the problems which may be associated with this type of physical deformity. this paper is designed to present a broad appreciation of the various aspects of the treatment of cleft lips and palates. incidence and cause the incidence of cleft palate in the white race is approximately one in 800, the incidence being less in negroes. the figures among investigators vary from' 1 in 665 to 1 in 2,000. the causes of this deformity are not fully understood, heredity still being the most significant cause. german measles in early pregnancy is another possible causative factor. malnutrition in animals has in instances produced clefts. varieties of clefts a cleft lip is the result of the non-fusion of the medial nasal process with the lateral nasal and maxillary process during the 6th to 7th week in utero. a cleft palate results from the lack of fusion of the palatine processes with each other and the nasal septum. palatal fusion is usually completed at the end of the 4th month in utero. thus it can be seen that a teratogenetic factor present between the 7th week and 4th month can be responsible for a cleft. also, the timing of the onset of this teratogenetic influence and its duration during this period, determines the type of cleft, since fusion of the palate is from anterior to posterior. a cleft through the alveolar ridge does not necessarily pass through the incisive suture2; clefts are present before the ossification of the maxillary elements. the variety of clefts is infinite, but for practical purposes veau's1 classification is the most acceptable. type i is a cleft of the soft palate or uvula. type ii is a cleft of the hard and soft palate extending no farther forward than the incisive foramen. the vomer in this type may be suspended from above and free from each side, or attached to either side. type iii is a complete uni-lateral cleft of the alveolar arch and hard and soft palate (usually associated with uni-lateral cleft lip). in this type the vomer is usually attached to that side of the palate which is opposite to the cleft in the alveolus. type iv is a complete bi-lateral cleft of the alveolus and hard and soft palate. the vomer and premaxilla are not attached to either side, but are suspended from above (associated with bi-lateral cleft lip). it is noteworthy that the width of the cleft is greater, and the shortening and deficiency of the lateral elements are usually more marked in types i and ii than in complete clefts involving the alveolus. effects of the deformity the cleft palate child for obvious reasons is a person who finds social and mental adjustment most difficult. the facial disfigurement is unpleasant and attracts the attention of all. adults react with obvious pity, especially the parents, who, in an effort to compensate for the deformity, may allow the child to become selfish. this adds to the difficulties of the child when associating with playmates. children are apt to remark publicly in order to embarrass or ridicule those less favoured than themselves. the nasal speech is imitated by taunting playmates, with the result that the child, in order to disguise his conspicuous nasal tone, may adopt compensatory tongue and lip movements which result in incorrect speech habits. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society september he dreads ridicule, is sensitive to the extreme and often adopts an attitude of open rebellion. he is unhappy, uncomfortable and at a serious disadvantage in this competitive world. a cleft extending through the alveolus frequently results in one or more missing or abnormal teeth and a cleft through the palate invariably affects the pattern of eruption and correct positioning of teeth. in the event of a tooth bud being split, two abnormally small teeth or supernumeraries are formed. the hygiene of the nose and throat is impaired by a cleft in the hard or soft palate thus acting as a predisposing cause to a variety of inflammatory lesions which are readily communicated to the middle ear via the eustachian tube. all inflammatory lesions of the upper respiratory tract should thus be promptly treated as negligence can cause a loss of hearing. surgical treatment the cleft lip can only be effectively treated by means of surgical repair, but the cleft of the palate may either be treated by means of surgical procedures and/or by means of a prosthetic appliance. the former has, in latter years, become the method of choice due to vastly improved surgical techniques and the great advances in anaesthesia, both of which have contributed to the low mortality rate and improved end result. surgical treatment has, however, not supplanted the use of all prosthetic appliances. the earlier routine practice of indiscriminate placing of surgical wires through the cleft segments, thereby forcing or holding the jaw together, has fallen into complete disfavour. the effects of cicatrix contraction is also well recognised, so much so that at one period surgeons were moved to postpone closure of the hard palate until school age. this postponement was based on the assumption that any operations on the hard palate would retard the normal growth process of the maxilla. investigations by means of cephalometric roentgenograms have shown that people with cleft palates exhibit the same stability of skeletal pattern as individuals with no such deformity. the jaw relationships remain the same as do their rates of growth. investigations have shown that there are cases in which the hard palate has been operated on at an early age without any deleterious effect to the ultimate growth of the jaws, but this area involves one of the principle growth centres of the face and interference with the regional blood supply, particularly the posterior palatine vessels, tends to inhibit growth. brodie and slaughter 3 studied the results of 1,349 surgically treated cases and concluded that surgery does inhibit normal growth. they claim that the interference is directly proportional to the amount of injury to growth centres and to the diminution of blood supply to the parts concerned. they strongly suggest that there be no unwarranted trauma to soft tissue and no interference with its blood supply. the object of the modern surgical procedures employed for the treatment of cleft palates is to restore continuity between the two portions of the hard and soft palate and still to preserve a normal palatal and dental arch form with a flexible soft palate of sufficient length to effect velopharyngeal closure. this closure is essential for normal speech. most children with type iii or iv clefts have their initial operations during their first three months of life. in a uni-lateral cleft, type iii, the adjacent parts of the upper lip are brought together in order to establish the integrity of the lips thereby establishing the proper labial forces on the denture. this lip musculature will assist in bringing the displaced anterior segments of the upper jaw together. repair of the bi-lateral cleft lip, type iv, is usually carried out in two stages: the one side is first repaired and at a later date in the first year the opposite side is treated in a similar manner. the soft palate is usually also operated on during this early period. some surgeons prefer to repair the hard and soft palate simultaneously, while others prefer to postpone the operation on the hard palate. the premaxillary segment is kept intact whenever possible. the approach and differences of opinion to this problem are beyond the scope of this paper. prosthetic treatment the earliest problem confronting the prosthodontist may be the construction of a temporary obturator to facilitate feeding prior to the surgical treatment. the patient with a type iii and iv cleft usually has one or more teeth absent in the line of the cleft. rudimentary teeth or those in poor occlusal relationship to the rest of the arch are frequently removed; a prosthetic appliance in these cases carries the necessary dental replacements. this appliance is essential for aesthetic reasons, the production of proper speech sounds and the support of the labial musculature. one of the most important functions of a prosthetic appliance is, however, the substitution for an ununited hard or soft palate by means of a suitable material when surgical treatment is contraindicated. an efficient prosthetic appliance will subserve speech, improve the appearance and assist the patient in obtaining some degree of oral resonance. it is necessary for the prosthetic r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society appliance to cover the extent of the cleft laterally and antero-posteriorly. it should be sufficiently long antero-posteriorly to enable comfortable contact with the pharyngeal muscles during contraction. care is taken to see that these muscles approximate the appliance with sufficient pressure to establish a proper air seal. too forceful a contact on the other hand will cause an irritation of the muscular tissue and also tend to dislodge the appliance. the prothesis should be just wide enough to permit easy and comfortable soft palatal movement without loss of contact of the tissues with the prosthesis during speech activities and deglutition. the prosthesis is vaulted as much as is anatomically desirable in order to provide the oral spaciousness necessary for free tongue movements. reduction of this oral space by an insufficiently vaulted palate will create further unnecessary speech obstacles. after orthodontic treatment a dental prosthesis is frequently used to act as a retaining appliance, particularly in the prevention of a collapse of the buccal segments. a prosthetic appliance should be comfortable, of light weight, tissue tolerant, smoothly finished to facilitate cleaning, and above all, correctly planned. since alterations become necessary during growth, it is desirable that the appliance be easily modified by extensions or reductions to it. acrylic westdene products (pty.) ltd. specialists in medical literature recommend— speech problems of children edited by wendell johnson, ; price 38/3 this book offers authoritative information on speech disorders; it points the way to a full, normal and well-adjusted life for uncounted thousands of children. the chapters express the viewpoints and reflect the praotical experience of the authors and of 20 other experts. order your copy now! johannesburg: 23 essanby house, 175 jeppe street. cape town: 408 grand parade centre, castle street. durban: 66/67 national mutual building, smith street. pretoria: 210 medical centre, pretorius st. resins satisfy most of these requirements and is consequently the material of popular choice. orthodontic treatment the orthodontist often only sees the cleft palate patient after the lips, soft palate and frequently hard palate have been repaired. surgery to the lip re-establishes the bucco-labial muscular band which exerts a constricting effect on the two maxillary segments. pre-surgically the segments of the maxilla are often widely separated. after surgical treatment of a type iii cleft the patient presents an arch form of a fairly typical nature. there is an approximation of the alveolar segments anteriorly, the smaller segment usually being over-rotated to become contained within the premaxillary alveolar section of the larger segment. there is some concomitant narrowing of the posterior portion of the palate. the effect on the maxillary alveolar process is such that it is now lingually positioned in relation to the mandibular arch, particularly in the anterior region. the deficiency of the upper jaw frequently gives the patient an appearance of having a lower jaw prognathism. laminagraphy4 has shown that the moulding action of a repaired lip and palate is not necessarily confined only to the alveolar bone; the arrangement of the tral>eculae of the whole maxilla is affected. in many cases an approximation is established between the turbinate and the nasal septum. this has been postulated as a possible explanation for the distorted nasal speech sounds described as hyponasality superimposed on the hypernasal quality. this construction of the maxilla may also be present before any surgical intervention of the palate. the prime objective in orthodontic therapy in these cases is the establishment of correct arch form and occlusion and thus the improvement of appearance, mastication, deglutition and speech. to achieve this the orthodontist has to expand the maxillary arch in such a way that the maxillary teeth will articulate correctly with the mandibular teeth. the effect of an expansive force ιο the cleft palate differs radically from the effects in noncleft cases. ordinarily, expansion in the non-cleft involves the movement of individual teeth through bone, but the same expansive forces, when applied to the cleft palate does not move the teeth through bone per se, but actually moves the two segments of the maxilla apart. tooth movement is minimal, the entire maxillary bone housing the teeth becomes repositioned. for this reason the type of expansion in cleft palate cases frequently improves nasal ventilation in that the turbinates are moved away from the nasal septum, particularly in the anterior region. the expansive phase can be completed r p n i f l l v i n 9. tri λ mnntli<: r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society september in the type iv clefts of the lip and palate, the buccal segments are usually collapsed and the premaxillary segment is located well ahead of the maxillary arch. it is suspended from the cartilagenous nasal septum which scott v claims to be one of the cardinal growth centres for the face. this anterior segment consists of teeth and alveolar bone; no muscular tissue is attached. the size of the segment varies, the larger the segment the more teeth it accommodates.. surgical repair of the lip envelops this protruding maxillary mass retarding its forward growth and this helps to establish a more normal relationship with the two buccal segments of the maxilla after they have been expanded. the teeth in the anterior segment are usually severely lingually inclined and malformed and may have to be extracted. further orthodontic treatment is usually necessary at about 12 years of age when most of the permanent teeth have erupted, to align the teeth and establish an occlusion. a prosthetic appliance with the necessary tooth replacements is frequently used as an orthodontic retaining device. early treatment is almost always indicated because the deformity is less in younger patients and results are obtained more rapidly. if the maxillary teeth are aligned and repositioned, the mandibular arch tends to accommodate itself to the upper before severe mandibular deviations are allowed to become established. unfolding the maxillary arch at an early age allows for the normal function and development of the jaws, prevents the perverted forces of crossbites to distort the growth pattern and aggravate the condition. the psychological advantages must naturally not be overlooked. it may be correct to assume that early correction will assist with the normal development of the functions of speech. if the deformity is slight it may be advisable to postpone treatment until the eruption of the permanent teeth. apart from cleft-lip-palate problems, cases are frequently complicated by antero posterior skeletal dysplasias. these are readily diagnosed by means of cephalometric roentgenology and will require additional treatment for these malocclusions. problems of the speech therapist the first problem a child with a cleft palate encounters is that of feeding. with co-operation and advice this is not an unsurmountable problem. the second is one of making himself understood through the use of speech. this is more complex in that the ability of being able to express one's thoughts in a language takes time, and involves the ultimate in neuro-muscular control of the speech mechanism. speech is usually not established until the end of the second year of life, but the actual sounds used in speech are acquired very much earlier. speech is at first confined to vowel sounds, but at about the 10th week the lips and tongue begin to play their part and consonant sounds make their appearance. the change from vowel sounds to the use of sounds which more nearly resemble language is a gradual and complicated process involving observation, initiation, imitation, repetition, association of ideas and images, ability to understand and the precise control of the speech apparatus. the development of speech in the cleft palate child is for a time similar to the normal, but since the oral and nasal cavities remain undivided, there is a distinct nasality in all sounds. a form of speech which has a marked deviation from the normal begins to develop. the child is becoming accustomed to the sound he hears himself producing. unfortunately these abnormal auditory images are associated with the normal speech sounds he hears and is attempting to imitate. the child is invariably not conscious of the fact that his speech is any different from that of his associates. some time later he begins to realise that it is only with difficulty that he is able to make himself understood. frequently it is when his playmates ridicule his attempts at speech that he begins to appreciate that he is different from others. faulty speech habits are being developed and these habits are difficult to eliminate in later years. the production of normal speech in the cleft palate patient is the ultimate objective of the speech therapist. the ability of the therapist to attain this goal depends largely on the success of the earlier treatment. the degree of nasality is usually proportionate to the efficiency of the palatopharyngeal sphincter, nasality being absent in the highly successful surgical cases having a completely functional soft palate. in cases where an anatomical deficiency remains and normal physiological function is impossible, the speech therapist is obliged to make the best possible use of the existing musculature by means of exercise. perseverance and co-operation of the patient are of major importance in the establishment of modified speech habits. obturators are fitted to patients where surgery is either contraindicated or where secondary operations are deemed inadvisable. this will assist in the separation of the oral and nasal cavities. where neither of the above alternatives are possible, the only course a therapist can adopt is to train the patient to direct his voice and breath in a manner that the maximum amount possible passes through the mouth and minimum through the nose. the intelligence quotient of a patient likewise plays an important role in effecting a good result; the more intelligent the child, the greater the liker ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society lihood of the therapist achieving a result. if intelligence is decidedly below normal there is every likelihood that the child's speech will remain defective despite the ability of the patient to correctly articulate individual sounds and also to seal the nasal cavity from the pharynx thereby preventing any nasal escape. where operative procedures have not been entirely successful, the prognosis in the mentally retarded is poor; the ability to co-operate and/or concentrate make the elimination of faulty habits particularly difficult, if not impossible. since children with cleft palates are prone to inflammatory lesions of the pharynx, the middle ear is frequently infected. infections of the ear, if neglected, frequently lead to deafness, which in turn hinders speech re-education. in many instances the patient is obliged to make use of a hearing aid. the age at which the cleft palate is repaired is important. in cases operated on between one and two years of age, before the production of consonant sounds are learnt, provided the result of surgery is successful, normal speech may be attained without the aid of special training. the bad habits acquired before operation may. not have had a chance to obtain a persistent hold. these defects are not lost immediately after the operation, the change and loss of nasality is gradual. the longer the operation is delayed, the more difficult it becomes for the patient to rid himself of his acquired faulty speech habits. with the compliments of clinical emergencies (pty.) ltd. 207 jeppe street. phone 22-0458. conscientious effort on the part of the therapist and patient alike is usually sufficient to overcome most of these difficulties. where an incompetent palato-pharyngeal closure persists, the patient is talight to make the best possible use of the artificial and anatomical structures at his disposal. among other factors that influence the result of speech therapy is the personality of the patient, his or her environment and of course the therapist patient relationship. discussion and conclusion the problems involved in the treatment of a cleft palate individual are obviously vast. only the surgical, prosthetic, orthodontic and speech problems have been mentioned, but the psychologist, otorrhinolaryngologist, dentist, pediatrician and sociologist may also have to play an important part in the treatment of these unfortunate individuals. a cleft palate patient is, therefore, not a dental problem, neither is he a surgical problem, nor is he a speech problem or a psychological problem; he is an individual and a personality and should be regarded as such. the most efficient and satisfactory method of treating every variety of cleft palate is through a group of people adequately prepared by close professional association conducting a truly integrated and co-ordinated care programme. ideally, the patient should be jointly examined by this clinical team and the total care programme fully outlined. full records should be kept and the patients recalled from time to time and the problems jointly re-evaluated. these multi-professional teams are not easily organized, neither are they easily managed. a suitable alternative however, particularly in a region or country where no such clinical team exists, would be the formation of a discussion group, where a single case is examined and the records studied by all. each member should be called upon to present his aspect of the case and thei difficulties he expects to encounter. in this way each member of the group would glean an understanding or appreciation of his colleagues' difficulties and likewise the possibilities of therapy. bibliography 1. veaux, v. d i v i s i o n palatine, p a r i s 1931. masson e t cle. 2. jacobson, alex. e m b r y o l o g i c a l evidence for t h e none x i s t e n c e of t h e p r e m a x i l l a in man. off. j . d . a s s . of s.a., 10, 189, 1955. 3. slaughter, w a y n e , b. and brodie, allan g. facial clefts and t h e i r surgical m a n a g e m e n t in view of recent research. j . of p l a s t i c and reconstructive s u r g e r y , vol. 4, no. 4, j u l y 1949. 4. subtelny, j a c o b d. and brodie, allan g. an a n a l y s i s of orthodontic e x p a n s i o n in uni-lateral cleft lip and cleft palate patients. am. j . orth. vol. 40, sept. 1954, pp. 686. 5. scott, j. h. brit. d . j . 79, 278, 1953. 6. scott, j. h. and brash, j. c. a e t i o l o g y of i r r e g u l a r i t y and malocclusion of t h e teeth, ed. 2, london 1956, d e n t a l board of t h e u n i t e d k i n g d o m . 7. p r u z a n s k y , samuel and l i s , e d w a r d f. cephalometrlc r o e n t g e n o g r a p h y of infants; sedation, i n s t r u m e n t a t i o n and research. a. j o u r n a l orth. vol. 44, march 1959, pp. 159. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abstract introduction methods ethical considerations results and discussion conclusion acknowledgements references about the author(s) amisha kanji department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, south africa katijah khoza-shangase department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand, south africa citation kanji, a., & khoza-shangase, k. (2016). feasibility of newborn hearing screening in a public hospital setting in south africa: a pilot study. south african journal of communication disorders, 63(1), a150. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i1.150 original research feasibility of newborn hearing screening in a public hospital setting in south africa: a pilot study amisha kanji, katijah khoza-shangase received: 20 jan. 2016; accepted: 08 may 2016; published: 21 july 2016 copyright: © 2016. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract objectives: the current pilot study aimed to explore the feasibility of newborn hearing screening (nhs) in a hospital setting with clinical significance for the implementation of nhs. context-specific objectives included determining the average time required to screen each neonate or infant; the most suitable time for initial hearing screening in the wards; as well as the ambient noise levels in the wards and at the neonatal follow-up clinic where screening would be conducted. method: a descriptive, longitudinal, repeated measures, within-subjects design was employed. the pilot study comprised 11 participants who underwent hearing screening. data were analysed using descriptive statistics. results: the average time taken to conduct hearing screening using otoacoustic emissions and automated auditory brainstem response was 18.4 minutes, with transient evoked otoacoustic emissions taking the least time. ambient noise levels differed between wards and clinics with the sound level readings ranging between 50 dba and 70 dba. the most suitable screening time was found to be the afternoons, after feeding times. conclusion: findings highlight important considerations when embarking on larger scale nhs studies or when planning a hospital nhs programme. current findings suggest that nhs can be efficiently and effectively conducted in public sector hospitals in south africa, provided that test time is considered in addition to sensitivity and specificity when deciding on a screening protocol; bar recognised personnel challenges. introduction early detection of hearing loss is the initial stage to any early hearing detection and intervention (ehdi) programme and is conducted by means of newborn hearing screening (nhs). the implementation of nhs programmes has evolved over the years, particularly in developed countries, with universal newborn hearing screening (unhs) being the recommended and most widely practiced approach in these contexts. however, nhs programmes in south africa have not been standardised, nor uniformly implemented nationally, with documented differences between the public and private healthcare sectors. these differences in the approach to screening, screening protocols and the overall lack of nhs services in south africa have all been documented in national surveys (meyer & swanepoel, 2011; theunissen & swanepoel, 2008). a variety of objective screening measures may be employed within a nhs programme. these include otoacoustic emissions (oaes), automated auditory brainstem response (aabr) and a combination of oae and aabr (berninger & westling, 2011). whilst oaes are simple, fast and cheaper, they provide limited assessment of the auditory system. oaes are also negatively impacted by vernix, middle ear fluid and ambient noise (choo & meinzen-derr, 2010). in contrast, the aabr provides more information regarding the auditory system and provides better detection of auditory neuropathy in infants. however, the aabr requires more knowledge and expertise to conduct, which limits the number of screening personnel who can utilise it. it is also typically more costly and requires a longer test time to conduct (choo & meinzen-derr, 2010) when compared to oaes. these are all factors that may influence the implementation of a comprehensive screening programme in different contexts – particularly in resource constrained contexts such as south africa. limited studies have documented the test time during screening that has excluded preparation time. the entire screening activity (from start to finish) is what defines the success of a programme and is important when positioning notions of feasibility. the technical focus on the time related to the test procedure is also not aligned with models of clinical feasibility studies and is especially not aligned with the current study context (public healthcare) as is the grander intention of this pilot study. cebulla and shehata-dieler (2012) concluded that conducting aabr screening, using the mb11 beraphone, reduces test time when compared to other portable screening equipment. various ehdi position statements recommend the use of different screening measures for different screening contexts. the joint committee on infant hearing (jcih), for example, recommends the use of oae or aabr for infants admitted to well-infant nurseries and aabr for infants admitted to the neonatal intensive care unit (nicu) (jcih, 2007). the hpcsa (2007) position statement, on the other hand, recommends the use of aabr for infants admitted to the nicu and oae for screening during immunisation visits at primary healthcare clinics within the south african context. review of the literature, however, indicates that the ideal hearing screening measure is yet to be defined (guastini et al., 2010), with various protocols currently being used in different contexts. such diverse recommendations call for further exploration and definition of feasible and context-specific screening protocols. such exploration would have to include deliberating on the entire ehdi process, including follow-up as well as diagnostic or interventional components of ehdi. aspects such as coverage rates, referral rates, recording and tracking systems, as well as follow-up rates, are crucial in such feasibility studies. the feasibility of screening protocols may also include consideration of ambient noise levels within the screening environment, particularly as ambient noise levels may influence oae screening outcomes. presence of undetected and unmonitored high ambient noise levels may result in higher referral rates or false-positive rates (olusanya, 2010), which not only influences the efficiency of the nhs programme but has cost implications for the programme. in addition, it can cause unnecessary emotional distress for parents due to false-positive hearing screening results (poulakis, barker & wake, 2003). there are a number of studies that have explored the feasibility of nhs. some of these studies have focused on coverage rates, referral rates and tracking systems associated with unhs (ng, hui, lam, goh & yeung, 2004; pisacane et al., 2013). other studies have focused on the feasibility in terms of the type of screening protocol employed (kumar et al., 2014; qi et al., 2013). feasibility studies are commonly performed in many clinical areas. these studies usually commence with some small-scale investigation or pilot study to determine the feasibility of conducting a larger scale study. pilot studies also assist in assessing feasibility in terms of the process, resources, management and scientific aspects such as treatment efficacy (thabane et al., 2010). the current pilot study focused on the process involved with screening procedures and logistical aspects as it aimed at exploring the feasibility of nhs in a public hospital setting, thereby assisting in guiding a larger scale study. the south african public healthcare setting faces significant challenges with regard to numerous factors including burden of disease, limited resources, issues of linguistic and cultural diversity influencing the healthcare provision, socio-economic status of the population served etc. all these factors have been documented to play a major role in the provision of public healthcare throughout all levels of care, which includes audiology services. it is within this public health sector context that the current study was located. methods objectives of the pilot study determine the average time required to screen each neonate or infant. determine the most suitable time for the initial hearing screening in the wards. determine the ambient noise levels in the wards and at the neonatal follow-up clinic. the time taken to conduct the hearing screening is critical when screening services are planned in any environment, but more so in an under-resourced environment where personnel:infant ratio is unfavourable and time limitations exist, irrespective of whether the screening personnel consists of a highly skilled audiologist, speech therapist, trained nursing staff or volunteers. within the south african context, the general shortage of healthcare personnel, with a high demand to capacity ratio for audiologists, poses a significant challenge for feasibility of implementing nhs. furthermore, the fact that there are still no promulgated minimum standards of training for professionals other than audiologists to conduct nhs creates further challenges. it is for these reasons that it becomes important to establish normative data around time taken to conduct screening as this evidence allows for approximations of how many newborns or infants can reasonably and accurately be screened within a given time period. the second objective of the study was to ensure that the hearing screening did not disrupt other nursing duties and/or ward rounds. determining the appropriate time for hearing screening could also inform the researcher as to possible times at which caregivers would be present in the wards in order to obtain informed consent. such information is crucial for the sustainability of a nhs programme and for ethical practice. it would allow for efficient information counselling when caregivers are present. by identifying suitable times for screening, it is also possible to describe less suitable times that may impact on reliability, validity and sustainability, which is of particular relevance in the current context where efficiency and expediency are key. the third objective of this particular pilot study was to ensure that the ambient noise levels did not exceed those suggested in literature. measurement of the ambient noise levels in the screening environments also allowed the researcher to make the necessary adaptations to minimise these, which in turn contribute to the reliability and validity of hearing screening results. establishment of noise levels in paediatric wards and clinics has implications for newborn and infant care. it has the potential to guide best practice by limiting noise exposure to newborns and infants through appropriate noise assessment and monitoring programmes (neille, george & khoza-shangase, 2014). research design a descriptive, longitudinal, repeated measures, within-subjects design was employed. this research design was deemed appropriate as it made use of the same hearing screening measures on the same group of participants over time and allowed for all the necessary test–retest, between and within subject comparisons to be made (schiavetti & metz, 2006). participants a total number of 15 babies were discharged or being queried for discharge during the pilot study period. of these 15 babies, 11 caregivers provided consent, three caregivers did not volunteer to participate and one baby was discharged before the initial hearing screening could be conducted. all 11 participants were booked for a follow-up hearing screening on the same day during their neonatal follow-up (6 weeks after discharge) to ensure that there were no false-negative screening results, and for assessing the appropriateness of implementing screening within the neonatal follow-up clinic should the neonate have missed in-ward screening as is often the case within the south african context. neonates admitted to the nicu or high care wards (after birth) and transferred to ‘step down’ wards once medically stable, and for whom consent was obtained from the caregiver were included in the study. neonates or infants who were previously discharged, returned home and were then readmitted to any of the wards were not enrolled in the study at the time of initial, in-hospital hearing screening. procedures hearing screening was conducted using the accuscreen oae/aabr screener. transient evoked otoacoustic emission (teoae) screening was conducted (frequency range: 1.5–4.5 khz). this was followed by distortion product (dp) otoacoustic emission (dpoae) screening that was conducted using the most comprehensive protocol on the machine with a 4/6 frequency pass criterion. aabr screening was conducted at the default level of 35 dbnhl using high forehead, cheek and nape of the neck electrode placements. the average time required to screen each neonate or infant was established by recording the time taken to screen using all three screening measures and included the time taken to ensure appropriate probe fit and acceptable impedance. these times were available when downloading the data from the screening equipment and were recorded by subtracting the starting time from the time of completion. these recordings were then added and divided by the number of screening sessions in the study. the most suitable time for the initial hearing screening in the wards was determined through field observation as well as through informal discussions with relevant personnel in the respective wards. the researcher engaged in collaborative discussions with consultant paediatricians and head nursing staff in the wards to establish the most suitable time for the initial hearing screening. ambient noise levels in the wards and the neonatal follow-up clinic were measured using a low-cost quest sound level meter, which is affordable, readily accessible, easy to use and has applications that include community and audiometric measurement or analysis. the sound level meter was placed at the caregiver’s bedside and the maximum noise level was measured and recorded for each screening session. the initial hearing screening was conducted over a period of a week. following completion of the initial hearing screening, results were explained to caregivers, and follow-up appointments were provided. follow-up hearing screening was conducted approximately four to 6 weeks after the first phase in the morning, on days that coincided with neonatal follow-up clinic at the respective hospitals. data analysis data were captured onto an excel spreadsheet and were analysed using descriptive statistics. measures of central tendency such as the mean were used to summarise data. data were also summarised using tabulated descriptions (schiavetti & metz, 2006). ethical considerations ethical clearance was obtained from the university medical ethics committee (m1211103) and permission was also obtained from relevant authorities at the research sites where the study was conducted. informed consent was obtained from caregivers, and anonymity was ensured by assigning numbers to the completed data collection sheets. the home languages of the caregivers of participants were not recorded; however, all caregivers were able to speak and understand english and therefore did not require the use of an interpreter. results and discussion from the initial hearing screening, six participants presented with an overall bilateral refer result for dpoae, four presented with a bilateral refer for aabr, and seven participants presented with an overall bilateral refer result for teoae. three participants presented with a bilateral pass result for dpoae, one participant presented with a bilateral pass result for teoae and a bilateral pass result for aabr was present in two participants. a unilateral pass result for dpoae was present in two participants, with three participants presenting with a unilateral pass result for teoae and aabr (table 1). aabr could not be completed on two participants as they were restless and difficult to calm (figure 1). the high referral rate on one or more of the screening measures is consistent with literature that has also indicated a higher referral rate at the initial hearing screening in comparison with the repeat hearing screening (chen et al., 2012; colella-santos, hein, de souza, do amaral & casali, 2014). referral rates have also been documented to decrease with an increase in age (khoza-shangase & harbinson, 2015; van dyk, swanepoel & hall, 2015). figure 1: outcomes of hearing screening protocol employed during data collection. table 1: initial hearing screening result for each measure per participant. of the 11 participants, six attended neonatal follow-up clinic with five participants having undergone a second hearing screening. the caregiver of the one participant had left the clinic following consultation with the paediatrician. two of the five participants presented with pass results on all screening measures and were subsequently booked for behavioural audiometry at 6 months corrected age. both participants attended the 6-month follow-up and presented with visual reinforcement audiometry results that were within normal limits. one participant underwent a third hearing screening due to incomplete results at the follow-up screening. pass results were obtained for all screening measures, but there was no attendance at the 6-month follow-up. two participants obtained bilateral refer results on both dpoae and aabr measures and were subsequently booked for a diagnostic abr. however, both participants did not attend this follow-up appointment. these findings highlight the poor follow-up return rate in nhs programmes, which has been a widely reported challenge in both developing and developed contexts. the poor follow-up return rate in the current pilot study is consistent with findings from a community-based screening programme in nigeria where more than half of the participants did not return for the second screening even though services were offered free of charge (olusanya & akinyemi, 2009). these findings highlight the need to explore contextual factors (other than cost) that influence follow-up return rate, such as the influence of socio-economic status, cultural and linguistic factors within the south african context. description of participants in pilot study the pilot study comprised 11 participants. two of the caregivers of participants were zimbabwean, and the remaining nine were south african. with regard to ethnicity, all 11 participants were black african, with nine of the 11 participants being female and two being male. the ethnic profile of participants is reflective of the national estimates of the general south african population as well as the differences in access to, and use of public versus private healthcare facilities. black africans constitute approximately 80% of the total population in south africa and predominantly make use of public sector health services (statistics south africa, 2013a, 2013b, 2014). the average gestational age was 30 weeks, with six participants being classified as very low birth weight, one as low birth weight and two as extremely low birth weight. one participant had a normal birth weight of 2880 g, and for another participant the birth weight was not recorded in the file (table 2). table 2: description of participants. none of the participants had been admitted to the nicu, but all had a prolonged hospital stay. the mean stay in high care was 7 days, with a longer stay in the kangaroo mother care (kmc) ward for an average of 14 days. three of the 11 participants underwent phototherapy due to neonatal jaundice. with regard to retroviral disease (rvd) exposure, two participants were rvd exposed, seven were unexposed and the rvd status of two participants was unknown. medication considered to be ototoxic was administered to eight of the 11 participants during their hospital stay. these ototoxic drugs consisted of gentamycin, amikacin and vancomycin, with gentamycin having been the most frequently administered drug in seven of the participants. time required per screening measure for each neonate the average time to complete teoae screening was one minute per ear. dpoae screening time was slightly longer with the average time of two minutes per ear, and aabr screening time was similar to dpoae with an average time of two minutes per ear (one minute, 55 seconds). the average time taken to complete the hearing screening with all three measures was 18.4 minutes as this included obtaining good probe fit and ensuring that participants were calm when conducting each screening measure (table 3). table 3: time taken to complete each of the screening measures at the initial screening in minutes and seconds. the researcher observed three factors that influenced the time taken to complete each of the screening measures. the first factor was the state of the newborn or infant, the second factor was the signal-to-noise ratio for oae screening and the third factor was the eeg for the aabr. preferably, the newborn or infant should be resting quietly in a bassinette or crib, and if needed may be held (asha, 2015). the best results were obtained for babies who were awake but calm, or for sleeping newborns or infants with a good signal-to-noise ratio on oae screening and a good eeg, as these factors resulted in a shorter test time. with regard to oaes, particularly dpoaes, the larger the dp to noise ratio, the quicker the criteria for the dp are met, which in turn results in a shorter test time (hall, 2000) and a good eeg as an influencing factor is consistent with reports that the newborn’s state of consciousness influences the time to complete the assessment (sena-yoshinaga, almeida, côrtes-andrade & lewis, 2014). these findings have important implications for screening programmes as they indicate both the time requirements as well as possible influencing factors, which, if managed, would lead to the success of a screening programme, especially in an under-resourced environment. current findings are consistent with those reported in a number of studies that have reported aabr test time to be longer than when assessing using oaes. this has been based on the premise that additional time is required for electrode placement (berg, prieve, serpanos & wheaton, 2011; meier, narabyashi, probst & schmuziger, 2004; norton et al., 2000). however, longer test time for aabr has also been noted in the study by van dyk and colleagues that employed teoae and the mb11 beraphone, which does not require placement of disposable electrodes (van dyk et al., 2015). contrary findings have been reported in the study by sena-yoshinago and colleagues who reported an aabr screening time close to that of teoae, with a mean assessment time of 32.9 seconds (sena-yoshinaga et al. 2014). these authors further concluded a shorter test time for newborns that were in stage 1 of consciousness (deep sleep, no movement, regular breathing). most suitable days and times for screening from the discussions with nursing staff, it appears as if the afternoons were better suited for initial hearing screening, as ward rounds were usually completed. it was easier to identify babies who were going to be discharged or being queried for discharge during this time. the noise levels were significantly less because student training in academic hospitals usually occurs on ward rounds during the morning. the most suitable time for screening appeared to be between feeding times when babies were generally comfortable, satisfied and sleeping. these are ideal conditions for hearing screening where objective measures are utilised and sleeping is the preferred neonatal state of arousal. babies were reportedly weighed on tuesday and friday mornings at the one hospital, unless they weighed < 1500 g, in which case they got weighed daily. based on the initial aims of the pilot study, these were therefore identified as the most suitable days for hearing screening at this research site. to prevent babies from being omitted from screening on days that the researcher was not in the hospital, it seemed best to screen babies on the days they got weighed (e.g. tuesdays and fridays in this case). babies at the second hospital were weighed everyday but mondays and thursdays were recommended days by paediatricians for the initial hearing screening from 11:00 onward following completion of ward rounds. these recommended differences amongst hospitals highlight the need for audiologists to explore the site-specific, routine care offered at the respective hospitals prior to the implementation of nhs. caregivers were usually present at all times in two of the wards at the first hospital and were always present in the kmc ward at the second hospital. difficulties were sometimes experienced in the high care wards at both hospitals due to a lack of accommodation for mothers in these wards. some of these mothers had to be admitted to another ward for medical care whilst their babies were cared for by nursing staff in these wards. ambient noise levels ambient noise levels should also be taken into consideration when deciding on the most appropriate time to conduct hearing screening, especially in the wards within a hospital-based context. the noise levels not only have an effect on the screening time, but also on the sensitivity and specificity of the objective screening measures employed within an nhs programme (salina, abdullah, mukari & azmi, 2010). although khoza-shangase and harbinson (2015) have suggested that in order for oae measures to be reliable, ambient noise levels should not exceed 50 dba to 55 dba of noise; current sound level readings ranged between 50 dba and 70 dba in the kmc wards (table 4), and screening in the current sample was deemed possible. table 4: sound level recordings within the screening environments at the first hospital. the average sound level was 59.6 dba, making screening in these wards possible. some authors have reported that accurate oae screening results are obtained when sound levels do not exceed 65 dba – 68 dba (olusanya, 2010; salina et al., 2010). screening was not conducted in the high care ward as there were only two babies during the pilot study that were being discharged. the one caregiver left the hospital prior to having her baby’s hearing screened, and the second baby was transferred to a different ward where the screening was then done. however, this baby was full term with a normal birth weight, and the caregiver was being referred to a nearby clinic for follow-up after discharge. sound level readings ranged between 48 dba and 60 dba in the kmc ward at the second hospital. the high care ward at this hospital was not a suitable environment for hearing screening due to multiple cribs per cubicle, noise generated by alarms on incubators and a high volume of medical and nursing staff conducting routine care and training of medical students during the course of the day. similar noise sources have been reported in a study conducted at private and public sector hospitals in south africa (neille et al., 2014). it was therefore decided that babies in cribs would be tested in the last empty cubicle used for storage in the high care ward or an empty cubicle in the kmc ward, where ambient noise levels could be controlled. the average sound level in the screening environment at the neonatal follow-up clinic at the first hospital was 57.25 dba. these sound levels were lower at the second hospital and ranged between 40 dba and 50 dba as the follow-up hearing screening was conducted in the audiology department due to a lack of available consulting rooms in the clinic. the audiology department was a short distance away from the clinic, which allowed for fairly easy access to the participants. limitations it is acknowledged that the data used to describe the context are site-specific and need to be explored on a larger scale for similar contexts. it is further acknowledged that the limited sample size in the current pilot study prevented the generalisation of findings. these findings need to be considered when planning for larger scale nhs studies or when planning a hospital-based nhs programme. larger scale studies on the implementation of a nhs programmes are necessary in the south african context. conclusion findings suggest that nhs can be conducted in public sector hospitals in south africa, provided that test time is considered in addition to sensitivity and specificity when deciding on the screening protocol to be adopted. these factors have even more relevance within the south african context where limited resources are an important consideration. furthermore, high sensitivity and specificity of the adopted screening protocol can possibly facilitate use of non-audiologists in screening programmes where minimum standards of training screeners have been adhered to. implementation of an nhs programme over a 12-month period, with consideration of test time, ambient noise levels, test sensitivity and specificity, may provide more information regarding the feasibility in a hospital setting. it is recommended that the impact of ambient noise levels on the time taken to complete screening as well as the screening outcomes be further explored in future studies. current findings seem to indicate ambient noise levels at the edge of the desired maximum limits within the screening contexts; therefore, careful monitoring of noise levels as part of any screening protocol is highlighted as important. careful attention to these factors would improve the efficacy of neonatal screening programmes within the high demand to capacity ratio south african audiologists function under. acknowledgements this work is based on the research supported in part by the national research foundation of south africa (unique grant no: 92673). competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships which may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions a.k. was responsible for conceptualisation, data collection, analysis and write-up of the paper. k.k-s. made conceptual contributions, supervised the pilot project and was involved in the write-up of the paper. references asha. (2015). expert panel recommendations on newborn hearing screening, retrieved from http://www.asha.org/topics/expert-panel-recommendations-on-newborn-hearing-screening/#6 berg, a.l., prieve, b.a., serpanos, y.c., & wheaton, m.a. (2011). hearing screening in a well-infant nursery: profile of automated abr-fail/oae-pass. pediatrics, 27(2), 269–275. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2010-0676 berninger, e., & westling, b. (2011). outcome of a universal newborn hearing screening programme based on multiple transient-evoked otoacoustic emissions and clinical brainstem response audiometry. acta oto-laryngologica, 131, 728–739. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/00016489.2011.554440 cebulla, m., & shehata-dieler, w. (2012). abr-based newborn hearing screening with mb11 beraphone using an optimized chirp for acoustical stimulation. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 76, 536–543. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2012.01.012 chen, g., yi, x., chen, p., dong, j., yang, g., & fu, s. (2012). a large-scale newborn hearing screening in rural areas in china. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 76(12), 1771–1774. choo, d., & meinzen-derr, j. (2010). universal newborn hearing screening in 2010. current opinion in otolaryngology & head and neck surgery, 18, 399–404. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/moo.0b013e32833d475d colella-santos, m.f., hein, t.a.d., de souza, g.l., do amaral, m.i.r., & casali, r.l. (2014). newborn hearing screening and early diagnostic in the nicu. biomed research international, 2014, 1–11. http://dx.doi.org/10.1155/2014/845308 guastini, l., mora, r., dellepiane, m., santamauro, v., mora, m., rocca, a., et al. (2010). evaluation of an automated auditory brainstem response in a multi-stage infant hearing screening. european archives of otolaryngology, 267, 1199–1205. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00405-010-1209-z hall, j.w. (2000). handbook of otoacoustic emissions. new york: thomson delmar learning. hpcsa. (2007). early hearing detection and intervention programmes in south africa, position statement year 2007. retrieved from http://www.hpsca.co.za/hpcsa/default.aspx?id=137 jcih. (2007). year 2007 position statement: principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. pediatrics, 120(4), 898–921. http://dx.doi.org/10.1542/peds.2007-2333 khoza-shangase, k., & harbinson, s. (2015). evaluation of universal newborn hearing screening in south african primary care. african journal of primary health care and family medicine, 7(1), 1–12. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/phcfm.v7i1.769 kumar, a., chowdareddy, n., sodhi, k., dalal, s.s., venkateshwar, & chander, r.b. (2014). feasibility of two staged newborn hearing screening and identification of risk factors for hearing loss other than those included in high risk registry (hrr) given by joint committee on infant hearing (jcih). international journal of medical science and public health, 3(3), 257–260. meier, s., narabyashi, o., probst, r., & schmuziger, n. (2004). comparison of currently available devices designed for newborn hearing screening using automated auditory brainstem and/or otoacoustic emission measurements. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 68, 927–934. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2004.02.008 meyer, m.e., & swanepoel, d. (2011). newborn hearing screening in the private health care sector – a national survey. south african medical journal, 101(9), 665–667. neille, j., george, k., & khoza-shangase, k. (2014). a study investigating sound sources and noise levels in neonatal intensive care units. south african journal of child health, 8(1), 6–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.7196/sajch.676 ng, p.k., hui, y., lam, b.c., goh, w.h., & yeung, c.y. (2004). feasibility of implementing a universal neonatal hearing screening programme using distortion product otoacoustic emission detection at a university hospital in hong kong. hong kong medical journal, 10(1), 6–13. norton, s.j., gorga, m.p., widen, j.e., folsom, r.c., sininger, y., cone-wesson, b., et al. (2000). identification of neonatal hearing impairment: evaluation of transient evoked otoacoustic emission, distortion product otoacoustic emission, and auditory brain stem response test performance. ear and hearing, 21(5), 508–528. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/00003446-200010000-00013 olusanya, b.o. (2010). ambient noise levels and infant screening programs in developing countries. international journal of audiology, 49, 535–541. http://dx.doi.org/10.3109/14992021003717768 olusanya, b.o., & akinyemi, o.o. (2009). community-based infant hearing screening in a developing country: parental uptake and follow-up services. bmc public health, 9, 66. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2458-9-66 pisacane, a., auletta, g., toscano, f., errichiello, m., barrier, f., riccardi, p., et al. (2013). feasibility and effectiveness of a population-based newborn hearing screening in an economically deprived region of italy. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 77, 329–333. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2012.09.019 poulakis, z., barker, m., & wake, m. (2003). six month impact of false positives in an australian infant hearing screening programme. archives of disease in childhood, 88, 20–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1136/adc.88.1.20 qi, b., cheng, x., en, h., liu, b., peng, s., zhen, y., et al. (2013). assessment of the feasibility and coverage of a modified universal hearing screening protocol for use with newborn babies of migrant workers in beijing. bmc pediatrics, 13, 116. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2431-13-116 salina, h., abdullah, a., mukari, s.z.m., & azmi, m.t. (2010). effects of background noise on recording of portable transient-evoked otoacoustic emission in newborn hearing screening. european archives of otorhinolaryngology, 267, 495–499. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00405-009-1080-y schiavetti, n., & metz, d.e. (2006). evaluating research in communicative disorders. (5th edn.). boston, ma: pearson education. sena-yoshinaga, t.a., almeida, m.g., côrtes-andrade, i.f., & lewis, d.r. (2014). neonatal hearing screening with automated auditory brainstem response: using different technologies. audiol commun res, 19(1), 19–24. http://dx.doi.org/10.1590/s2317-64312014000100005 statistics south africa. (2013a). gender statistics in south africa, 2011. retrieved n.d., from http://www.statssa.gov.za/publications/report-03-10-05/report-03-10-052011.pdf statistics south africa. (2013b). use of health facilities and levels of selected health conditions in south africa: findings from the general household survey, 2011. retrieved n.d., from http://www.statssa.gov.za/publications/report-03-00-05/report-03-00-052011.pdf statistics south africa. (2014). mid-year population estimates 2014. retrieved n.d., from http://www.statssa.gov.za/publications/p0302/p03022014.pdf thabane, l., ma, j., chu, r., cheng, j., ismaila, a., rios, l.p., et al. (2010). a tutorial on pilot studies: the what, why and how. bmc medical research methodology, 10, 1–10. http://dx.doi.org/10.1186/1471-2288-10-1 theunissen, m., & swanepoel, d. (2008). early hearing detection and intervention services in the public health care sector in south africa. international journal of audiology, 47, s23–s29. http://dx.doi.org/10.1080/14992020802294032 van dyk, m., swanepoel, d., & hall, j.w. (2015). outcomes with oae and aabr screening in the first 48h-implications for newborn hearing screening in developing countries. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 79(7), 1034–1040. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.ijporl.2015.04.021 o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y understanding the hard of hearing child by a. l. behr, d.ed., ph.d. the great american philosopher william james once said: "all life comes back to our speech—the medium through which we communicate." experience and research alike demonstrate that human feelings have their sources in language (1). it is largely through language that the feelings of one person towards another are created. provocative language of the mildest sort influences profoundly personal and social relationships. an investigation with some eighty children proved that those who were mildly though repeatedly prohibited and scolded, developed a measure of hostility to the experimenter. they were unwilling to do him a personal favour, or to repeat a simple task, or to stay for play with interesting toys. on the other hand, those who were spoken to kindly and encouragingly reacted quite differently. they showed a greater willingness to do the experimenter a favour, to repeat a task and to stay when invited to play with new toys (2). it must, therefore, be clearly realized that in a language situation, the words connote more than their conventional meaning. words have a personal meaning as well, and it is the latter that has the determining value. for example, listen when someone says: "no!" is the word convincing? does it mean "no" to the person speaking or to the person listening? is it deceptive and does it really mean "perhaps"? the answer lies in the relationship felt between the speaker and the hearer (3). the meanings which children give to words spoken by their' parents and teachers are not always the accepted customary meanings, but are modified and altered in a special 'way by the personal feelings which arise through the use of those words. the author s experience with normal hearing children leads him to believe that these children always attach an implicit meaning to words and spoken language in general, as distinct from the conventional adult meaning, and that it is very often difficult for the adult to realize that what he means by a word, phrase or sentence, and what the child means, is not the same. if adults are not always certain what the hearing child means by a word, how much more difficult does it not become with hard-of-hearing children? many teachers concerned with hard-ofhearing children, in their anxiety to teach these children conventional language, often lose sight of the child as an individual. the child is forgotten, but the language development is regarded as paramount. the author's plea is that an understanding of the child as an individual must come first. everything else is subservient to this. if the child is understood, his language will be understood, for his language is merely a vehicle for formulating his needs, his experience, his intentions, and his thoughts. being hard-of-hearing does not have the same meaning in every child, as the degree and the type of impairment of hearing vary. one type of impairment may limit the distance at which the child can hear conversation to 2 or 3 feet. moreover, if such a child can do lipreading, he may give the impression that he can hear at a considerably greater distance. another type of hearing impairment causes the child to hear some sounds well and others in distorted form. on the whole, the hard-of-hearing child, unlike the profoundly deaf child, has been seriously neglected both medically and socially (4). this may be due to the fact that the handicap is not readily discovered. sometimes five or six years elapse before the hearing impairment becomes known (5). the behaviour and social responses of the hard-of-hearing child have been misunderstood by his parents, his teachers and his playmates. too often he has been considered stupid in school, has failed repeatedly in his class work, has been accused of paying no attention and showing no interest in his lessons. at home he has been scolded for not attending when spoken to, for disobedience, for failure to carry out instructions that he did not hear. he has been left out of games by his playmates, laughed at for making irrelevant replies to the questions they have put and, unable to understand the reason for their derision, he has become a lonely, dissatisfied, often unhappy and emotionally disturbed person. unable to make a proper adjustment with his playmates, he becomes unusually retiring and dependent upon his parents. thus, impairment of hearing in a child may start a series of disturbances which can seriously affect his ability to develop normally. while attention may be directed to him ber ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y a r l cause of his hearing difficulty, consideration must first be given to him as >an individual, growing physically, mentally, emotionally and socially. the early detection of hearing loss is extremely important. numerous cases of children brought to the hearing clinics for aural rehabilitation need not have reached that point in hearing impairment had adequate measures been taken early on. in one of the states of the u.s.a. a hearing conservation programme was initiated. all children on entering school in that state were subjected to a medical examination and those found to suffer from chronic diseases of the ears and upper respiratory tract were given prompt treatment. this treatment saved these children from possible permanent hearing impairment. in order to detect the hard-of-hearing child as soon as possible and to prevent possible hearing impairment in children with diseases of the' ear or respiratory tract, the author makes a plea for the appointment of audiometrists, who would make the rounds of the schools and examine all children who enter school for the first time. children in need of treatment would then be referred to an otologist. a recent study conducted at columbia university on 38 hard-of-hearing children between the ages of 13 and 18 years, disclosed that the adjustment to wearing a hearing aid was a rather difficult process both from the technical and the emotional point of view (7). young children make the easiest adjustment to their handicap, particularly children in the age group 3 to 6 years, and this is one important reason for early case finding. yet, even when a successful adjustment is made in childhood, as this study shows, the period of adolescence with its numerous problems, may be particularly difficult and results in a refusal to wear the aid. the child's motivation may be personal or the result of the attitude of his parents and his friends or all of these. personal and emotional factors may thus emerge leading to conflicts which may interfere with the acceptance of treatment. once the hard-of-hearing child has been discovered he is subjected to an intensive programme of speech audiometry, auditory training, lipreading, language comprehension and the like. but, often, in our enthusiasm to help him overcome the handicap, we forget the child as an individual. bringing speech and language to the hardof-hearing child is not enough. . we should concern ourselves with the whole child, with the child as a unique and distinct personality. educating the hard-of-hearing child involves more than an intellectual process. it involves, too, his emotional and social adjustment. since the child's speech and language mirrors his personality, an understanding of his personality will make us understand what is implicit in the language he uses. 1. dollard, j. and others.—"frustration and aggression." (yale university press, 1930.) 2. johnson, μ. w.—"language and children's behaviour." the elementary school journal, university of chicago, november • 1955. p. 141-145. 3. johnson, m. w.—"verbal influences on children's behaviour. (ann arbor, michigan university press, 1945.) 4. sutherland, d. a. and miller, m.—"rehabilitating the hard-of-hearing." (the child, october 1944 u.s. government printing office, washington 25, d.c.) 5. ibid. 6. lesser, a. j.—"some principles in the development of services for children with hearing im pairment." (the journal of speech and hearing disorders : federal society agency, u.s.a., june. 1950, 15, 101-105). 7. gates, a. t. and kushner, r.e.—"learning to use hearing aids : a study of factors influencing ths decision of children to wear hearing aids.' (teachers' college, columbia university, new york, 1946.) η boek vir u! d r a m a en spreekkuns in die skool juin du toit prys 18/posvry. vanwee die steeds toenemende belangstelling in spraakleer en voordragkuns, in toneelspel en dramatisering, in skoolkonserte en kunswedstryde, het die behoefte aan deskundige leiding op hierdie gebied al hoe ernstiger geword. hierdie werk van juin du toit, lektrise in drama aan die toneelskool op worcester, voorsien aan 'n groot behoefte. dit behels die beginsels in verband met voordragkuns, toneelspel, dramatisering, mimiek, spreekkoorwerk, spreekkuns, liggaamshouding, debatte en openbare redevoering en verstrek ook 'n uiigebreide reeks voorbeelde en oefeninge wat nuttig deur die onderwyser aangewend kan word. hierdie is een van die beste (indien nie die beste nie) handleidings vir skole op hierdie gebied, oorsese publikasies ingesluit. skryfster het hiermee pionierswerk verrig, waarvoor die onderwysprofessie haar dankbaar sal wees. daarby is die werkie van 'n netjiese en duursame band voorsien. bestel by: afrikaanse pers-boekhandel joubertstraat 76, johannesburg r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) page ten journal of the south african logopedic society sound games for dyslalias the practical application of van riper's theoretical chart. by joan streit, b.a. log. van riper1 advocates a nine step programme in the correction of defective articulatory sounds. the nine stages are as follows: 1. cannot make the sound nor discriminate it from its error even when word pairs are pronounced by another individual. 2. cannot make the sound but can hear the error in another's speech. 3. can make the sound in isolation but only after strong stimulation by teacher. 4. can make the sound in isolation without requiring stimulation. 5. can use the sound in nonsence syllables in all positions. 6. can use the sound in isolated words when careful. 7. can use the sound in careful speaking of prepared sentences. 8 can use the sound habitually in swift unemotional speech. 9. can use the sound habitually,., in swift emotional speech. . ... using this outline as a basis, an attempt has been made to construct games which can be used for motivation and stimulation in the implimentation of these steps·. the speech teacher in a public school situation finds herself confronted with the problem of handling a number of dyslalic children simultaneously so that therapy takes place primarily in a group situation. since the time is not available to establish rapport with each individual child, the speech therapist must construct an atmosphere wherein this can be established through play and pleasant speaking situations. keeping in mind the scientific approach as advocated by van riper, the following games have been devised for each individual stage, in order that they may provide (a) play and pleasant speaking situations. (b) an adequate group situation. stage 1. leave out — only for testing purposes. stage 2. (a) the card game. this was originally bought as a game called "speed' which includes picture cards of aeroplanes, trains, ships and cars. to each vehicle is then assigned a sound e.g. for a thetacism group the sounds are: aeroplane-th, ship-f, train-d, and carvoiced-th (f and d are used as they are most commonly substituted for th); for a sigmatism group the sounds are: aeroplane-th, ship-f, train-s, and car-z; and finally for a rhotacism group they are: aeroplane-r, ship-g, (afrikaans), train-1, and car-w. in this game, the pictures of all the 'planes, ships, trains and cars are placed in a pack. for this stage, the teacher holds the pack and raises the top card. should this be a ship, she says "f" and the first child to recognise that this is the ship sound and says "ship" is rewarded with the card — and so on through the pack. the child with the most cards at the end is the winner of t i e game. in this way the children are learning to discriminate the correct and wrong sounds. (b) finding the sound. an article is hidden by the therapist. when the child is near the hiding place, she says "th, r, or s" and she says "f, w, or th" when far from the object. (c) the hidden window game. the therapist hides behind a picture of a window so that her mouth is obscured. the children in front of the window attempt to pick out the correct sound from the sound series which, she makes. if they make an error, they can open the window to make sure. it should be noted that the correct sound is presented in sequences of similar and different sounds. a charm is rewarded to the first child to notice the correct sound. the child with the most charms at the end of the game is the winner. stage 3. (a) the train game. this game consists of railway tracks drawn in the shape of a figure 8 with different coloured stations at different points and a cardboard train. each child then chooses his station and each has a turn at pushing the train to the accompaniment of the therapist saying the sound to be learned, stopping at each station to refuel with water or coal. this is devised to teach the sound in isolation. (b) hiding the cards. the cards (with the sounds already given them in game (a) of stage 1), are hidden away by the therapist and when each is found, the child says the appropriate sound to the teacher in order to win the card. stage 4. (a) the card game — same as game in stage 2 (a) but this time the children join in. each child has a, turn in picking up the top card but being careful to say only the appropriate sound, and the rest guess'which picture was chosen. if the wrong sound is given e.g. f for th, they forfeit by replacing that card into the pack plus one of those which they have already won. (b) the train game — the game is the same as stage 3 (a) but is now continued with the .children advanced to saying the sound themselves. stage 5. (a) the ladder game. the material consists of paired objects all of which entails an object climbing r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society page een a ladder in order to reach a goal e.g. a little charm of humpety-dumpety climbs the ladder to get to the top of the wall, a charm of sailor sam climbs a ladder to get into a ship. each step of the ladder is a vowel sound and the charm represents a consonant, thus making a nonsense syllable. when the charm lands on a step he blends witn it e.g. humpty-dumpty equals s he blends on each step and becomes — soo, ahsah, etc. (b) the ping-pong game. for this game the pingpong table is drawn with people at each end. the ball is a counter. the object of this game for two to play and win a point for each correct saying of the nonsence syllable which the therapist says first. if said incorrectly, the opponent gains an extra point. the greatest number of points determines the winner. these games provide the amount of stimulation required to keep a child's interest while attempting to form the new habit through nonsence syllable exercises. stage 6. (a) secrets. to establish the new sound in words, this game is played by drawing on separate slips of paper, pictures of words containing the sound in all positions e.g. for the th sound — three, thumb, thief, path, birthday-cake, etc. each person takes one, the remainder being left in the box. the object of this game is for each person to ask the one on his left "have you got three, thumb, thief, etc.?" and so on through the list until each picture is discovered. one may not ask more than one object in one turn. (b) climbing the house. this is based on the game of "snakes and ladders". a large house is drawn with ladders leading to and from two windows on the top floor. in conjunction with this game the book "speech through pictures"2 is used. each child has a turn at throwing a dice and should they throw, for example, a five, that person has to say correctly what each of the five 'th, r or s" pictures to which the therapist points. if one picture is said incorrectly, then the person can only move four steps instead of the five which he threw. i (c) the thinking game. this game is also played / with the book "speech through pictures".2 the book is opened at the page containing the sound to be learned. one childj thinks of a specific picture on the page and the others, question him until they guess the right one. the person who guesses correctly, merits the next turn. questioning consists of "are you thinking about?" — thumb, thief, etc. the answer has to be "no, i am not thinking about the thimble, thumb, etc?" stage 7. (a) the games of climbing the house and ladders (same as stage 6 (b) and stage 5 (a) are played also for this stage, substituting nonsence syllables and words by sentences from "work and practice" book.3 the ladders are therefore still being used as motivation for exercises. stage 8. (a) nursery rhyme game. for this stage, the therapist uses the rhyme of "this is the house that jack built' with appropriate pictures to illustrate each new character e.g. "this is the flour that lay in the house that jack built". thus using the "th" sound in swift unemotional speech. for sigmetisms pictures of "simple simon" are used. (b) finger plays. another idea at this stage, is to make use of the numerous finger plays learnt from nursery schools such as "cherry stone", "incy-wincy spider', "five straight soldiers"4 for sigmatisms, and " 1 humpety, thumpety", "fee, fi, fo, fum"5 for thetacisms, etc. the writer feels that finger plays help distract the attention from the speech sound being taught. (c) story game "th, r or s" objects may be placed in front of the children and they can be asked to tell a story about all the objects on the table. (d) "i went to market" and "i spy". other wellknown games are those of "i went to market to buy a thimble, a birthday-cake, etc." or "i spy with my little eye something with a s sound e.g. bracelet, socks, etc." stage 9. (a) dramatisations and conversational situations. this consists of letting uie children act out the nursery mymes, tinger plays and stories from nemoy-davis6 and thus letting ihem use the sound in swift emotional speech. (b) they should also be allowed to speak freely of the events of the day, etc. by the time stage 6 has been completed, it is often still necessary to allow the child to discriminate by saying contrasting sounds. the following game is used in this procedure. the contrast game. pictures of words beginning with conflicting sounds e.g. f and th in the case of 1 hetacisms; or tli and s for sigmatisms are drawn the words can be sick and thick, sink and think, mouse and mouth or fin and thin, free and three, fort and thought, etc. each child has a turn in thinking of one and the rest have to guess what he is thinking about but have to point to the object which they mean so that the therapist can see if they mean the right name for that particular object. this game is played in exactly the same manner as the game in stage 6 (c). the writer has found the above games to have been of some value in relation to the aims as initially set out. they do not and are not planned to cover a complete speech therapy programme but are designed primarily to aid the speech therapist who find's it difficult to equip herself with expensive toys and motivating games. one cannot overlook the importance of parental and teacher co-operation or the value of regular practice but it is hoped that these games will find some small place in the speech therapist's programme. ' v a n r i p e r " s p e e c h c o r r e c t i o n . " p a g e 148 f m c c l a u s i a n d , miller and o k i e . " s p e e c h t h r o u g h p i c t u r e s . " " m c c u l l o u g h . " w o r k and practise b o o k . " ' b o y c e & b a r t l e t t . n u m b e r r h y m e s a n d f i n g e r plavs 5 b a r r o w s & h a l l . j a c k i n t h e b o x . ' n e m o y & d a v i s . c o r r e c t i o n of defective c o n s o n a n t s o u n d s . r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y the relation between speech therapy and psychotherapy by joan philips, b.a.. m.sc. any therapist dealing with human beings must establish a good relationship with the patient, and must know how to use this relationship skilfully in order to achieve success. to that extent it is true that in a sense every speech therapist must b e her own psychotherapist. the problem i s : how far can and should she consciously attempt to act a s such, and where should the dividing line be drawn? it is important to try and arrive at an answer to this question because in my experience some speech therapists tend to step right outside their province and to deal with psychological aspects of the problem for which they have neither sufficient training nor the kind of relationship with their patient which will enable them to adopt successfully the role of psychotherapist. we can, to begin with, distinguish between two main types of speech defects:— (1) mechanical defects, e.g., those due to organic conditions such a s cleft palate, failure to enunciate clearly, inability to pronounce certain consonants, faulty accent, etc. (2) defects involving, or related to personality difficulties, i.e., symptoms of disturbance within the personality which take the form of speech defects, e.g., stammering or refusal to speak. into this class will also fall certain cases of lisping, lalling and mispronunciation, which may, for example, b e part of a general tendency on the part of the individual to remain infantile. in dealing with the first type of defect the task of the speech therapist is more or less straightforward. primarily, her job is education or rej-education in the technique of good speech. nevertheless, she enters at once into a therapeutic relationship with the patient, and to some extent, if the patient be a child, with his parents. for one thing, the existence of the defect will in all probability already have produced secondary psychological difficulties, e.g., shyness, feeling of inferiority, reluctance to enter new situations. the therapist has to deal—though indirectly—with these aspects of the patient's personality a s well a s with his speech. it is also important that she understand a good deal about his background and past history, b e c a u s e the way in which she teaches will obviously be adapted and modified according to the sort of response she can elicit from her patient. when dealing with a child, it is also clearly important that she should be able to establish good rapport with the parents. to this end the speech therapist must have a good understanding of behaviour and its meaning, and of the technique of handling individuals in need of help. but she should use it a s the good teacher does—not obviously and self-consciously, but a s part of her general approach. she is teaching the patient, not analysing him, and her psychotherapy must at all times b e subordinate to her p e d a g o g y . true, removal of the speech defect will probably have psychological effects on the patient —in many cases there will be growth and change, a better adaptation to life—but these should come a s a result of the speech therapy a s such, not a s a result of attempts on the part of the speech therapist to use a determinedly psychological approach. let us turn now to the second type of defect—that involving or relating to the personality a s a whole. it is here that there is most confusion about the role of the speech therapist, and where, with the best intentions in the world, real harm may be done to the patient by an overenthusiastic speech therapist trying to take over the functions of psychotherapy proper. in the first place, it seems that there is a failure to appreciate that these defects are neurotic symptoms. now a neurotic symptom is a compromise solution of a conflict intolerable to the personality. an unconscious wish or impulse-seeking realisation is opposed by other forces within the personality, such a s conscience, fear of loss of love, fear of social disapproval. by producing a symptom the individual succeeds in keeping the conflict within bounds and giving expression to the warring elements. for instance, among the many factors contributing towards the development of a stammer are on the one hand, the unconscious wish to b e extremely aggressive verbally and on the other, the fear of this aggression and the need to repress it. the stammer represents the compromise. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y o c t o b e r speech is crippled (i.e., aggression is controlled), but at the same time the listener is irritated (i.e., aggression is expressed). the stammerer is punished by the disability (as demanded by his sense of guilt) but there is also some secondary gain (he draws attention to himself). this oversimplified description of a neurotic symptom should serve to show what a very economical and convenient device it is for the neurotic individual. small wonder, then, that he clings so tightly to his disability. every speech therapist must know from her own experience how deep can b e this reluctance to b e cured, despite the strongest conscious wish to return to normality. it must b e emphasised that while a physical defect may be surgically removed with very little effect on the body a s a whole, a neurotic symptom cannot b e dealt with except in relation to the whole personality. to try and "cure" it by removing it may well mean that you are simply blocking up one channel of discharge for the personality without providing an alternative outlet. here is an example of what i m e a n : a boy of fifteen came to me for treatment for continuous diarrhoea, which had been diagnosed by physicians a s psychogenic. two years previously he had started treatment by speech therapy for a severe stammer. the stammer had disappeared, and he had been discharged a s "cured," but shortly afterwards the speech symptom had been replaced by this new symptom of diarrhoea. now it should have been clear from the history of this boy, a s told by his mother, and from the very first interview with him that he w a s a deeply disturbed individual, and that the mere removal of the symptom of stammering would do little towards relieving his severe psychological problems. in these cases the onus is on the speech therapist to decide whether the speech defect is a comparatively simple, superficial symptom which is likely to respond to speech therapy a s such, illumined with psychological understanding, or whether it is a symptom of really deep psycho: • logical disturbance. if the latter, the patient should b e advised to seek psychological treatment in the first place. after improvement ot his psychological condition it may still be necessary for him to seek treatment for the speech defect. this would be a far safer course to adopt, and probably more profitable, too, b e c a u s e the adjusted individual can absorb instruction far more easily than the maladjusted. it may be argued that, in the light of these considerations, what the speech therapist needs is not less, but more psychology in her dealings with her patients. if more attention were paid to her psychological training and if she were taught more of the theory and technique of psychotherapy, would she not be better equipped to deal with all types of speech difficulties, including those which have deep psychological roots? in order to answer this we must examine more closely what the practice of psychotherapy implies and where it differs from speech therapy. we have seen that, in dealing with a conflict within himself, one of the devices adopted by the individual is to repress—i.e., banish into the unconscious—the undesirable thoughts, wishes and impulses. an unequal conflict then rages -within the personality—unequal, because one of the combatants is hidden, unknown. one of the objects of psychotherapy is to bring the repressed feelings and thoughts back again into consciousness. one way in which this is done is by re-living the original conflict within the therapy, only this time, feelings, wishes and thoughts which were originally directed towards the patient's parents, teachers, siblings, etc., are now directed towards the therapist, who represents now one, now another of these people at different stages of the treatment. when the patient discovers that he can bring out all the forbidden ideas and emotions with totally different results from those which he anticipated and dreaded, a change takes place in his attitude. once he is aware of both sets of conflicting feelings, he can make a conscious choice between them, instead of producing a neurotic symptom. he can accept some of his feelings and critically reject others, instead of having to repress them under pressure of fear, guilt and shame. this is again an oversimplification, but at least it may help to clarify what part the therapist must play in this struggle. he must b e prepared to accept all types of feeling from the patient, both positive and negative. his task is not to judge, nor to criticise nor to teach. the speech therapist, on the other hand, must definitely have an educative approach, and like all educators can only achieve good results if she is able to establish a clear positive transference. she needs the conscious co-operation of the patient. she cannot afford to work directly with the unconscious or to encourage free play of aggression, hatred, guilt, suspicion, etc. therefore, she cannot be both educator and therapist, r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o c t o b e r j o u r n a l o f t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y 1 5 any more than the psychotherapist can set out to cure a neurosis and at the same time teach good speech. the two functions must remain separate. attempts to combine them can only result in poor therapy and confusion for both patient and therapist. the speech therapist may here interject that she attempts nothing so deep in the way of psychological treatment, but limits herself to fairly superficial interpretations of the patient's behaviour and difficulties. in some cases, it is true, this may be harmless, since the interpretations have no effect at all. in that case, why make them in the first place? in other c a s e s they may touch upon and stir up anxieties which are far deeper than the therapist anticipates and which she then finds herself unable to deal with. or, a s in the c a s e of the fifteen year old boy, the attempt to probe into deep psychological causes is felt by the patient a s an attack, from which he tries to escape by giving up the speech symptom and choosing some other form of symptom. it is not my intention to decry speech therapy a s demanding less knowledge or less skill than psychotheraphy. the two professions, although closely allied, are completely separate. they rely on. different bodies of knowledge and different techniques of practice, and this must be borne in mind by clinicians of both professions if they are to achieve the best results in their respective spheres of work. vacancy for speech therapist the pretoria school for cerebral palsied children, p.o. box 1511, pretoria. phone : pretoria 20626 m i s s b e s s i e d e m b o 54 wingate masions, c/r., smit and nugget streets, 1 hospital hill, i johannesburg. phone 44-0860 typing and roneoing undertaken, especially theses'and students notes. d e a f ? 'the latest miracle of modern s c i e n c e » truly a product of the new electronic age, the a l l t r a n s i s t o r bonochord "tr3" •k no 'b' battery—just one standard cell! •k 500 hours' life from a carbon pen cell; the operating cost—a mere 2/6 a year (8-hour day)) * the tr3 is the smallest all-transistor aid in the world working from a standard pen cell—the size of a packet of book matches—yet robust and strong. w e i g h t — a mere 3 ozs.l . + crystal clear, true tone, simple to use, easy to conceal, comfortable and convenient. 4r purchaseable over 6 or 12 months, trade-in terms acceptable; there is a 12 months' guarantee of, trouble-free service under all normal conditions. do n't delay in hearing for yourself how much this can mean to you. there is no obligation—just phone or write for the name and address of our nearest agent. bonochord size i 2nd floor si well a s the emo t q of the .child. the p a χ , w h e n discuss a n y problem a n d is given a a n d n e e d , e 1 · · a t c s i a r e n o t e t b y a p a n e l p s y c h o l o g i c a l ° t h e c h i l d . a t reguo f d o c t o r s w n o r e e x a m i n e h e ^ ^ lar intervals during the y e a r ^ t q a n d l e e c h h a n d i c a p a r e the child s p , , , s i c a . e d _ n t h e important fac l oi= to c s p e e c h treatment. . speech evaluation. s p e e c h i e ^ ^ ^ h l ^ r s p a l s y m a y b e drrecfly due to ^ ^ ^ or they m a y occur, a s m } { a c t o r s . through o.ther o r g a n i c ο i o l l o w . f o r the purposes cf c i a « m c a a o n , ^ \ t r ^ g u s e f f l s c n b m e the s p e e c h s s f s e r w h k the children fall. τ d e l v e d . s p e e c h c e r e b r a l q a l s y h a v e foued to d e v e ο . ^ delay m a y b e o n e of the following : ' ( a ) s p e e c h is a n a c q u i r e d skill . t h e normal child h e a r s the words, i η frequent to h e a r a mo,ner , a y · w • ^ don't talk to him much he a o e ? η understand, so v™at r e a l l i y the child with c e r e b r a l j e n ° e x d e r i e n c e , n e e d s more stimulation a n d mo e ^ d i _ the child m a y h a v e ^ g r o u n d g o * physical a n d mental de^e oprnent. ?„g slow rate of s p e e c h development (g) the child m a y b e mentally retarded. 2. c e r e b r a l p a l s y ^ ^ ^ * velops s p e e c h it may b e c h a ς o f jerky, indistinct s o u n d , d u e t o ^ j 'speech moving m a n j a w or t i m e s the m e n t i s severely u v u l a a r e so spastic a . j a n d λ d f f l s l o ^ u n s ^ i b i e to the 1 ί 5 γ o r a a n i c or structural defects. s p e e c h 3. u r g a n i c 01 rviuied children c a u s e d defects m a y occur m p a l , ea^cn by a n o m a l i e s of ° r £ u f i s t 0 n o m h e c.n.s. than ' ^ o s e c a u s e d b y o f the athetoid group. i n e t ϊ α η y γ defect in the sounds such often it involves h e a r or disc s s. sh, ch, ne c r ι t h e r e i o r e s g u i s s s ? s o ^ s c ^ r e c i l y f d produces them incorrectly or not at all. 5 stuttering. stuttering m a y o c c u r ^ n g cerebral-palsy children, p e r n a p s a» a re.ult o^ confused, c e r e b r a l d o m i n a n c e r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l o f t h e south a f r i c a n l o g o p e d c society . s p e e c h . due to spasmodic or uncontrolled breathing musculature, the breath is said to \-e "out of p h a s e . " the child may b e unable to direct the flow of air through the mouth dur. s p e e c h , or movement may b e so inhibited q s to cause' shallow breathing insufficient for normal speech. 8. aphasia. this defect is a n impairment of linguistic function due to d a m a g e of the speech centre a n d a s s o c i a t e d tracts. e i s e n s o n 5 s t a t e s : "those patients whose outstanding difi i c u l t i e s a r e in the comprehension of l a n g u a g e , c'ooken or written, may b e classified a s belonging to the receptive type. where the predominant difficulties consist of an impairment of ability in speaking, word finding, oral reading, spelling, or writing, the patient may b e classified a s a n expressive a p h a s i c . " very often it a p p e a r s a s if the child's lack of response to speech is due to deafness, but in an aphasic hearing is usually unaffected. if the child develops a certain amount of speech, sentence structure is distorted, " s m a l l " words are often left out, while writing is characterized by "mirror" formation of letters a n d words. the speech defects found at the school a r e enumerated in the right-hand column of the table on p a g e 13 (one child m a y h a v e more than one defect). speech examination. before speech therapy is begun, a thorough speech examination must be m a d e to determine the possible c a u s e or causes of the delay or defect in the speech. the treatment will depend on these factors. the examination will include : 1. a physical examination of the speech organs to discover a n y structural malformation. j 2. a test' of the ability of the child to move tongue, lips, uvula, j a w s , etc., for a d e q u a t e speech performance. 3. a test of all the speech sounds in initial, medial a n d final positions, using pictures or objects. from this phonetic inventory omissions, distortions, a n d substitutions of sound during speech c a n b e detected. 4. a crude hearing test is given and if possible an audiometer test. 5. force a n d direction of the breath, a s well a s whether it is in co-ordination with the act of speech, a r e noted. 6. sucking, chewing a n d swallowing a c t s are g a u g e d . 7. voice factors such a s pitch a n d quality a r e noted. 8. a recording of the child's speech is taken to determine understandability and, after intervals, to determine progress if any. recordings a r e v a l u a b l e for therapy, a s they provide excellent auditory stimulation. very often the child h a s no idea what his speech sounds like, and the recordings allow him to hear himself and his defects. therapy and techniques. the child who, from the a g e 2 j years onwards h a s little or no speech is treated for delayed speech. at all times the child is e n c o u r a g e d to m a k e sounds, to b a b b l e , to indulge in all manner of vocal play. he must be taught to watch the lips, hear the sounds a n d feel the placement a n d voice vibrations. s p e e c h must b e a p l e a s u r a b l e activity a s well a s a necessary one. for stimulation, toys, pictures, rhymes, songs, g a m e s and dramatizations a r e used, depending on the a g e a n d amount of speech the child already has. at the school there a r e regular singing periods apart from the speech lessons, in which the nursery group a n d the older groups a r e separately taken. the children sing a n d dramatize english and afrikaans songs, a n d from their obvious enjoyment of the whole procedure it is clear that they are being stimulated and motivated to vocalize. in this group, too, there is a certain amount of healthy competition, and e a c h one tries to sing well so i'hat he or she will b e chosen to be the "matrosie," or "little miss muffet." the mother must b e instructed to b a b b l e with the child, to talk, sing a n d read to him. while she is doing his physical exercises with him, she can rhythmically count or sing in time with the actions. defects in the limited speech at this stage a r e ignored. any attempt to vocalize or say a n e w word is praised. at the s a m e time it is important to strengthen the speech musculature. the organs of speech h a v e a primary function other than for speech. the act of chewing a n d swallowing employs the s a m e muscles a s those used for speech. therefore practice in b a s i c functions, such c s chewing, sucking and swallowing form an important part of therapy. the mother is instructed to encourage the child to eat "hard foods" a n d to swallow all liquids through a straw. this procedure is adopted at school, too. in this manner the muscles of speech a r e strengthened and drooling, so often found in cerebral-palsy children, is diminished. the "chewing-method" is successful in achieving "improvement of the functions of the mouth a n d speech organs, a s well a s of the voice."6· in d e l a y e d speech, all types of cerebralpalsy a r e similarly treated. in an atmosphere r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 j o u r n a l of t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d c society may of quiet, r e l a x a t i o n is practised, using a r a g doll to illustrate floppy l e g s a n d arms, or dem o n s t r a t i n g the difference b e t w e e n the ;eel of t e n s e m u s c l e s a n d soft muscles. later, when the d e v e l o p m e n t of s p e e c h is progressing, differentiation is m a d e b e t w e e n the types 01 c e r e b r a l p a l s y , so that a p p r o p r i a t e metnods of e x e r c i s i n g c a n b e given. strictly s p e a k i n g , though, there is not much difference in the m e t h o d s a d o p t e d for s p a s t i c s a n d athetoids. it h a s b e e n s a i d that " t h e r e a r e 3 w a y s to treat the s p e e c h of the s p a s t i c p a r a l y t i c : first, r e l a x a t i o n ' ; s e c o n d , r e l a x a t i o n ; a n d third, rel a x a t i o n . " " t h e s a m e m a y b e s a i d to a p p l y to the t r e a t m e n t of athetoids. if the affected m u s c l e s c a n b e r e l a x e d , the s p a s t i c c a n b e t a u g h t to m o v e them in the d e s i r e d direction, a n d the a t h e t o i d c a n learn to control them. whilst in a r e l a x e d position the child is then given b r e a t h i n g e x e r c i s e s , first on his b a c k , a n d later in a sitting position. t h e child learns the " f e e l " of correct· b r e a t h i n g b y gently pushing the rib c a g e in a n d out. to promote g r e a t e r b r e a t h force there a r e m a n y g a m e s which the child c a n play, such a s blowing out a c a n d l e , b l o w i n g b u b b l e s , or blowing up a b a l l o o n . later h e c a n c o m b i n e b r e a t h i n g a n d a r t i c u l a t i o n b y vocalizing a sound, e.g., ' a h , a n d at the s a m e time s e e i n g how far a toy a e r o p l a n e will fly—the flight lasting a s long a s the vocalization. t h e child is a l s o t a u g h t to direct the air through the mouth. here a g a i n , blowing g a m e s a r e used, a s well a s e x e r c i s e s for improving tlie m o v e m e n t s cf the soft p a l a t e . t h e t o n g u e isj s u b j e c t to a g r e a t e r amount of involvement c a u s i n g s p e e c h d e f e c t s than a n y other of the articulator}o r g a n s . t o n g u e exercises, therefore, a r e given in the majority οι c a s e s . ih order to e n c o u r a g e the child to m o v e the itongue up, out, to the sides, or round the mouth, h e is instructed to lick a lollipop b y p l a c i n g it in a c e r t a i n position on the mouth, or outside the mouth. as a variation, a r u b b e r m a s k with a m o v a b l e t o n g u e is used, a n d the child h a s to imitate its actions. t h e r u b b e r " f u n n y m a n " is u s e d , too, for lip exerc i s e s to stretch the lips into a smile;_ to pucker them or o p e n them into a n " a h " position. with the u s e of g a m e s , j a w a n d u v u l a exerc i s e s a r e similarly given. if the child u s e s c e r t a i n s o u n d s incorrectly o w i n g to functional or o r g a n i c c a u s e s , the sounds a r e practised, not in isolation, but in words. f o r "this purpose s c r a p b o o k s containing l a r g e pictures illustrating the particular words a r e u s e d for e a c h child. for e x a m p l e , if a child subsitutes "f" for " t h " in his s p e e c n , l a r a e illustrations of a thumb a n d a t h i m c i e are" p a s t e d in to a s c r a p b o o k . parents h a v e to help find the pictures a n d the child c a n p a r t i c i p a t e b y cutting them out a n d p a s t i n g them in (if his p h y s i c a l h a n d i c a p permits). another set of c a r d s roughly d r a w n b y the therapist a n d duplicating the pictures provide m a n y a n d v a r i e d g a m e s to stimulate further p r a c t i c e on the sound. t h e " f e e l " of the sound on tne child's h a n d often provides the c u e lor correct imitation a n d motivation, e.g., letting tne child feel the b r e a t h force a s the therapist rep e a t s a n explosive c o n s o n a n t . all physical e x e r c i s e s to g a i n m u s c l e strength a n d control, a n d s p e e c h sound e x e r c i s e s a r e done m iront of the mirror so that the child c a n m a k e u s e of all sensory stimuli, visual, auditory a n a tactile. t h e treatment of a p h a s i c s is a n interesting c h a l l e n a e to a n y s p e e c h therapist. it is a n a t t e r of stimulating the child through a u d i tory visual a n d k i n a e s t h e t i c m e a n s until a meaningful response is elicited. o n e oi the children at the s c h o o l , a mixed type of e x pressive-receptive a p h a s i a , responded d r a m a tically to t r e a t m e n t : it b e g a n b y the cmld holding a felt ball, noting its size, s h a p e , a n d texture a n d hearing s e v e r a l repetitions of the word " b a l . " at the s a m e time she w a t c h e a the formation of the word in the mirror. alter s e v e r a l w e e k s of c o n s t a n t repetition, s h e respeondc-d b y s a y i n g " b a l " when s h e s a w tne ball. from then on, s e n t e n c e s w e r e constructed around this word, a l w a y s dramatizing the action, e.g., "skop die b a l . " mew words w e r e then introduced until a n e x t e n s i v e v o c a b u l a r y w a s built up. t h e results w e r e far b e t t e r for the e x p r e s s i v e impairment than fcr the receptive. in treating the h a r d o f h e a r i n g child, lipr e a d i n g is taught and, t o g e t h e r with the visum c u e s , kinaesthetic a n d a g r e a t d e a l of a u a i t o r y stimulation is given. e a r p h o n e s a t t a c h e d t c the recording m a c h i n e a r e a n important a i d to stimulate h e a r i n g . t h e school a s yet d o e s not p o s s e s s a " t r a i n e a r . " in the one c a s e w h e r e a h e a r i n g a i d h a s b e e n p u r c h a s e d ior a hard-of-hearing child, the results w e r e disa p p o i n t i n g ; the p a r e n t s could not afford a r e l i a b l e set, a n d the child, owing to low intelligence, could not a d j u s t herself to > this a p p a r a t u s . "it is urgent that all d e a f c e r e o r c , p a l s y children of g o o d ability who h a v e resid u a l c a p a c i t y to benefit from the u s e of h e a r ing a i d should b e given auditory training from ' the b e g i n n i n g of their education.''^. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l of t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society results. what is the aim of the s p e e c h therapist in dealing with these children? evans^ says, "perfect s p e e c h is usually a n impossibility . . after all p o s s i b l e repair or help to the m e c h a nism h a s b e e n secured, aim for practicality rcther than normalcy. if the s p e e c h sounds ccceo'able e v e n if not h i g h g r a d e , it should ™ a c c e p t e d b y the clinician a n d the effort r x c i ^ d " in judging the results of nearly i ν oars' therapy, therefore, the question h a s not b e * n whether the child c a n now s p e a k normally but, rather, whether in the very slow oroce-s of developing or improving his speecn, certain a o a l s h a v e b e e n r e a c h e d a n d maintained. for instance, c a n a now lift his tongue into a position to s a y t ? c a n β now blow out a c a n d l e directing the flow οι air through the mouth? has c s t o p p e d drooling? is d correctly substituting " t h " m words instead of " f " ? it is extremely difficult to a s s e s s the results objectively. t h e following is a s u b j e c t i v e attempt to a s s e s s the children's progress at speech ; number speech improved delayed speech 7 cerebral-palsy b organic or structural defects hard-of-hearing. speech stuttering voice disorders breathing disorders a p h c s l c s ί i difficulties. the difficulties e n c o u n t e r e d in the s p e e c h therapy of 'children with c e r e b r a l p a l s y h a v e posed m a n y problems, most important of which a r e i relaxation a n d breathing. t h e therapist m a y spend months trying to r e l a x a spastic or athetoid in supine or prone position only to find that there is no c a r r y o v e r m a sitting or standing position, a n d certainly no carry-over during the a c t of s p e e c h . similarly, the child m a y learn a form of correct silent breathing, yet during s p e a k i n g it m a y b e quite "out of p h a s e , " or entirely i n a d e q u a t e for speech. in other words the c a r r y o v e r is often a b s e n t h e r e too. the therapist must query whether a spastic muscle c a n b e completely r e l a x e d , or whether an athetoid muscle c a n b e controlled. particularly during speech, when a r t i c u l a t o r a n d breathi ng muscles a r e in action, how n e a r to normal c a n the parts b e conditioned, so that a d e q u a t e s p e e c h c a n b e a c h i e v e d ? a h a n d m a n i p u l a t e d iron lung, used in the united s t a t e s for t e a c h i n g correct breathing, should b e tried here, a s well a s all the stimulating " g a d g e t s " for motivating speech. app a r a t u s h a s b e e n constructed which lights -up or rings bells when the child s a y s a particular word correctly. another important problem at the school is that of achieving an a c c u r a t e assessment c i the child's hearing ability. crude hearing te<=ts a r e not very r e l i a b l e a n d the results a r e subjective. tests using the pure-tone audiometer a r e difficult to u s e in a young child, since h e does not know what is required 01 him a n d h e tires easily. it is even more dnficult to a s s e s s the a c c u r a c y of such a test when given to children of sub-normal intellig e n c e . varying d e g r e e s of hearing-loss s e e m to b e relatively common a m o n g palsied chilaι 1 1 ι 2 2 1 5 3 total in group 13 10 1 2 2 7 15 3 ren a n d this aspect h a s b e e n sadly n e g l e c in e x a m i n a t i o n a n d therapy cmd for record purposes. a difficulty peculiar to south african therapist is that of l a n g u a g e . naturally, e a c h child is given therauy in his h o m e l a n g u a g e but sometimes there is a confusion of both langu a a e s in the home a n d the child h a s tnis a d d e d difficulty to c o p e with. also, although there is a wealth of s p e e c h sound material at h a n d in english, the therapist must compile all afrikaans material from m a g a z i n e s , boons, verse etc. an afrikaans phonetic s p e e c n b o o k ' w o u l d b e of great h e l p to s p e e c h therapists. the present classification of s p e e c h defects h a s not proved very satisfactory. d e l a y e d s p e e c h a n d c e r e b r a l p a l s y s p e e c h include too w ' d e a variety of defects. leather's r e c e n h v dublished classifications should b e of g r e a t e r v a l u e 'or diagnostic a n d therapeutic purposes a n d to gain a more o b j e c t i v e assessment ot results. with c e r e b r a l p a l s y there is no "full understanding of the implications of the injury to the total orgctnism."i° to i n c r e a s e the understanding, the g r e a t e s t co-operation is n e c e c s a r v between the s p e e c h therapist a n d the physiotherapist. indeed, fu.ll c o o p e r a n o n is n e c e s s a r y among all staff m e m c e r s of a school to co-ordinate p h y s i c a l a n d educational methods. summary. c e r e b r a l p a l s y is p r o l o n g e d a n d difficult, a n d in its treatment it is n e c e s s a r y for workers in v a r i o u s fields to pool their observations a n d e x p e r i e n c e . s p e e c h therapy is one of the primary n e e d s communication b e t w e e n individuals for social a n d e c o n o m i c n e e d s b e i n g so essential. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 j o u r n a l of t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society may experience of 41 children at the pretoria school for cerebral-palsy is described. types of cerebral-palsy a n d speech defects found in these children a r e detailed. methods a n d techniques in speech therapy a r e reviewed. results a n d difficulties a r e discussed. my thanks are due to dr. b. epstein, chairman of the board o! management of the pretoria scnool for cerebral-palsy, for his encouragement and assistance, and for allowing me cccess to files and recoras. references. 1 perlstein μ a and b a m e ^ η. e. (1952) : j. amer. med. assoc., 148, 1389. 2 pohl j. f. (1950) : cerebral-palsy. 2nd ed., p. 165167.' st. paul, minnesota: bruce publishing'co. 3. rutherford, b. r. (1948) : give them a chance to talk, 1st ed., p. 3. minneapolis, minnesota: burgess publishing co. 4 kastein, s. (1952) : notes on speech therapy, 1st ed., p. 7. british council for the v/elfcre of spastics. 5 eisenson, j. (1946): examining ior aphasia, 1st ed., p. 4. new york: the psychological corporation. 6. sittig, e. (1947) : j. speech -dis., 12, 194 (quoting froeschells). 7. evans, m. f. (1947): ibid., 12, 97. 8 ε wing i. r. and a. w. g. (1954) : speech and the deaf child, 1st ed., p. 127. manchester: university press. 9. leather, d. (1954): int.-j. phoniatry, 6, 38 (separatum). 10' clemons, e. s. (1953): j. s. afr. logopedic soc., special congress ed., p. 48 (johannesburg univ. witwatersrand). the conquest of stuttering c. van riper director, speech clinic, western michigan. college kalamazoo, michigan, u.s.a. western michigan college of education. for centuries the treatment of stuttering h a s wrecked itself on the rock of symptom avoidance. the various therapeutic methods^ used, relaxation, rate control, unusual modes oi speech, h a v e b e e n focussed on the s a m e goal which h a s b e t r a y e d e v e r y stutterer's own attempts to h e a l himself: the attempt to speak without stuttering. such c n effort carries within itself, even when successful, the seeds ot its own eventual failure. for a v o i d a n c e c r e e d s fear w h e n we flee from fear, w e magnify it. the situation a n d word fears so long conditioned in the adult stutterer c a n haralv ce e r a s e d by; such measures. according to modern teaming theory, anxiety conditioned responses never extinguish completely. o n e pairing of the shock with the conditioned stimuli restores them to almost full strengtn. and so we find the discouraging frequency of r e l a p s e s in stuttering therapy. moreover, much of the older methods did little more than to repress the symptoms. the powenul suggestion employed b y most therapists can ind e e d produce such repression temporarily, ^ut stuttering, like murder, will out ! w e may b e a b l e to hold down the coiled spring of tne disorder for a time, but so long a s it is intact a n d a s strona a s ever, it will eventually e s c a p e from our grasp. w e a r e but mortals with no ability to sustain a repression for long. no matter how confident we b e c o m e , existence will sooner or later c a u s e morale to efco. no environment, however favourable, will b e without its moments of trauma. to build fluency upon an attitude a l o n e is to use fiux instead of mortar for the foundation. ana. so at these i n e s c a p a b l e moments of e g o weakness, the fears invade our minds a g a i n , and the stuttering returns to haunt our lives. is there no w a y to exorcise this evil ghost whose strength seems almost of the supernatural? the psychoanalysts have tried a n d most of them confess failure since speech, their · healing tool, is itself affected. the myriad devices, methods a n d tricks which have b e e n used upon stutterers since the dawn of history give us little hope of s u r c e a s e from that direction. witchcraft and surgery, vocal training a n d hypnosis, in none of these have we found consistent effectiveness. our inability to c o p e with the severe stutterer after all these y e a r s still reflects discredit upon our profession. perhaps we h a v e b e e n working in the wrong direction. the stutterer does not need to b e taught how to talk normally. he already h a s that skill, a s much of his speech attests. suppose, instead of trying to k e e p him from stuttering with all of its attendant evils, we try to train him to modify his symptoms in the direction of fluency. the immense variety of stuttering symptoms suggests that among them there might b e a few types which society would not dunish. among them there should r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 43 development of semantic processes for academic language in foundation phase eal learners giselle meirim heila jordaan amy kallenbach meera rijhumal department of speech pathology and audiology, school of human and community development, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg correspondence to: h jordaan (heila.jordaan@wits.ac.za) the most significant issue for quality in education is ‘the all pervasive and extremely powerful influence of language which is unambiguously implicated in learning … and the need for pupils to have as good a grasp of the language of teaching and learning as possible’ (taylor, muller & vinjevold, 2003, p. 65). the purpose of this study was to track the development of some of the semantic processing skills underlying the development of english as the language of teaching and learning in grade 1 3 english additional language (eal) learners. the rationale is based on the widely acknowledged fact that language competence and proficiency are central to educational success (bashir, conte & heerde, 1998; hoff, 2005; owens, 2008; westby, 1994). one of the reasons for this is that literacy is inherently a language-based activity, rooted in well-developed oral language skills (linan-thompson & ortiz, 2009; scarborough, 2001; snow, tabors & dickenson, 2001). this study focuses specifically on semantics, which deals with the expression and understanding of meaning and refers to the mental dictionary of words or the lexicon (de villiers, 2004). significantly, semantic knowledge plays an important role in the acquisition of early literacy and comprehension skills (linanthompson & ortiz, 2009). therefore, research on the acquisition of these language skills in the south african context is critical, since low levels of literacy are currently of serious concern (howie, 2009; kgosana, 2010; tyobeka, 2006). in education, language proficiency involves more than the ability to communicate in everyday conversational contexts, but is specifically related to the use of language for academic purposes. cummins (2000, p. 67) defines academic language proficiency as ‘… access to and command of the oral and written academic registers of schooling’. academic language proficiency is not acquired as naturally as basic interpersonal communication skills and develops through exposure to formal education (cummins, 2000; hoff, 2005). this implies that educators should facilitate this development with explicit teaching strategies addressing the language implicit in various learning areas. the language skills that should be developed during the foundation phase and their importance for academic development have been well described (hoff, 2005; mclaughlin, 1998; owens, 2008). those pertaining to semantics include: rapid growth in vocabulary and conceptualisation, growth in knowledge of word formation processes, and the increasing ability to learn new words from context, a skill known as fast mapping (hoff, 2005). there is research to show that the acquisition of these skills can be facilitated through explicit teaching of vocabulary and assisting children to make use of context to acquire words (maynard, pullen & coyne, 2010). however, these skills may not be explicitly addressed because language for academic purposes as a distinct register is not always recognised. in addition, there is evidence to suggest that teachers in south africa are not only unaware of their responsibility to meet the language-related needs of learners but also lack the methodological skills to promote effective learning of academic language because they have not had the necessary training (mroz, 2006; o’connor & geiger, 2009; uys, van der walt, van den berg, & botha, 2007). however, the most compelling reason for investigating academic language development in the south african education system is that many learners must accomplish this in a second/additional language. as a result of our political history and the socio-linguistic influences operating within the country and by extension in education, the second language is almost always english (braam, 2004; de klerk, 2002; de wet, 2002; ntshingila, 2006). eal learners are effectively learning the language of instruction through the language of instruction (cummins, 2000), which may have an impact on their academic development. cummins (2000) has suggested that eal learners acquire basic interpersonal communication skills (bics) in about 2 years, while cognitive academic language (calp) skills are acquired in a period of 4 9 years, depending on the quality of english instruction they receive. regarding the language of instruction, research conducted internationally (cited in genesee, paradis & crago, 2004, p. 168), as well as in south africa (heugh, 2000; macdonald, 1990), has provided strong evidence to suggest that learners develop academic language proficiency more effectively in their home language or alternatively, in bilingual/ multilingual education, where teaching occurs in both the first and second languages. education policies in south africa strongly support the use of home language and/or bilingual instruction (e.g. language in education policy (liep), 1997), but in reality, the implementation of these policies has been slow (alexander, 2010; beukes, 2008; carstens, 2006; kamwendo, 2006). recently, there does seem to be a renewed will to implement home language instruction since the current minister of abstract it is widely acknowledged that language competence is central to educational success, primarily because literacy is inherently a language-based activity. vocabulary knowledge specifically plays an important role in the acquisition of reading comprehension skills. language in education practice in south africa is currently highly controversial, as the implementation of home language or bilingual instruction policies has not been achieved in many schools. the aim of this study was to investigate the development of language skills in foundation phase english additional language (eal) learners attending schools where english is the language of learning and teaching. a 3-year longitudinal investigation of the acquisition of some of the processes underlying language for academic purposes was undertaken using the semantics subtests of the developmental evaluation of language variation criterion referenced edition (seymour, roeper & de villiers, 2003). the results indicated that the majority of eal learners improved with increased exposure to english in the academic environment and by the time they were in grade 3, were performing at a higher level than english first language learners in grade 2. however, the effects of this protracted period of development on literacy attainment should be investigated. the significant individual variation in the learners’ performance has implications for assessment and instruction of eal learners and for the collaborative role of teachers and speech language therapists in the education system. keywords: academic language, eal learners, foundation phase education, semantic development 44 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 semantic development basic education has announced that from 2011, the language chosen by the learner as the language of learning and teaching (lolt) shall be taught as a subject, or as a first additional language, from grade 1 and not from grade 2, as is currently the case. the teaching of english would therefore occur alongside mother tongue instruction for those learners who choose english as the lolt. english will subsequently not replace the home language in the early grades (motshekga, 2010). however, the implementation of home language instruction and teaching of english in the foundation phase does not in any way absolve teachers of the responsibility for facilitating the learning of academic language. the principles are applicable to the teaching of any language. reasons for the hitherto lack of implementation of the liep include: the majority of south african parents believe that english is the language of empowerment and aspire to have their children educated in english (gules, 2005; kgosana, 2006); insufficient resources have been made available to give effect to home language and/or bilingual instruction (manyike, 2007; pandor, 2005); but more importantly the marked heterogeneity in the language backgrounds of learners and teachers, particularly in gauteng, results in the inevitable choice of english as the lolt (adler, 2001; granville, janks, joseph, mphalele, ramani, reed & watson, 1997; webb, 2004). table i reflects the heterogeneity in the language backgrounds of the grade 1 learners in the three schools participating in this study. these figures clearly show that the implementation of home language instruction would be problematic in this context. this raises an important question: how do foundation phase learners manage to acquire english for academic purposes? this study attempts to address this question, since there is limited research on the extent to which eal learners acquire the processes underlying academic language, and there have been few attempts to record their progress in the development of these skills over time. the study aims to reveal specific aspects that should be taught in this phase of education and to provide guidelines for the training of educators. the findings may also contribute to an understanding of what can reasonably be expected from eal learners in the foundation phase so as to formulate valid assessment standards for academic language. in this regard, the collaborative role of the classroom teacher and the speech-language therapist (slt) is relevant. slts have an intimate knowledge of the nature, development and functions of oral and written language. the slt may act as the school’s ‘language expert’ (owens, 2004), who assumes the roles of co-teacher and consultant, as well as direct service provider, and is fully integrated in the classroom. the slt is uniquely qualified to assist the classroom teacher in assessing each child’s level of functioning, analysing the language requirements of various curricular activities and materials, and developing intervention strategies. the slt could thus be a valuable resource in developing the academic language skills of the learners. the results of this study provide support for this collaboration, which is currently largely lacking in the south african education system (o’connor & geiger, 2009). typically, the evaluation of language is a difficult task, as language is multi-dimensional and not easily measured. teachers tend to evaluate learners according to criteria based on the content of their language and not the processes that underlie language. in this study, the processes underlying vocabulary development were assessed using the semantic subtests of the developmental evaluation of language variation criterion referenced edition (delv-cr) (seymour, roeper & de villiers, 2003). the delv-cr is an individually administered diagnostic test designed to identify language disorders in 4 9-yearold children. although it was constructed for use with children whose first and primary language is english and to identify language disorders in children regardless of whether they speak mainstream american english (mae) or african american english (aae), it was considered appropriate for use in the south african context for a number of reasons. first, it assesses the processes and properties of language that go beneath the surface structure to tap knowledge that is universal to all speakers of english. second, its focus on critical aspects of academic language makes it particularly valuable for the purpose of this study. third, it can be used with children aged 4.0 9.11 years and is therefore appropriate for children in the foundation phase of education who are generally 6 10 years old. the delv-cr edition is the culmination of many years of research and conceptual advances in the areas of language acquisition and communication disorders. it was field tested on 1 014 4 9-year-olds from working class backgrounds in all regions of north america. sixty per cent of the sample were speakers of aae and were matched for parental education level (high school and lower) to the mainstream american children in the sample. approximately one-third of the children at each age and in each dialect group were language-impaired. despite its american origin, the delv is considered to be highly appropriate for assessing the development of language for academic purposes in any context, because it is specifically designed to capture many aspects of language that are important for success in early schooling and the transition to literacy (de villiers, 2004). it provides a profile of strengths and weaknesses and therefore has implications for areas and methods of teaching and intervention. method aim the main aim of the study was to determine how foundation-phase eal learners acquire the semantic processing skills underlying language for academic purposes. design the study falls within the quantitative, descriptive paradigm, and is longitudinal in nature. a longitudinal design is considered to be the most appropriate method of obtaining information on the process of language acquisition, which is dynamic and influenced by a number of variables, all of which can affect performance on a single measure (de bot, lowie & verschoor, 2005). repeated measures over time are essential to obtain an accurate reflection of language abilities. the eal learners were therefore followed up over a period of 2 years and withingroup comparisons of their performance in grades 1 and 2 and in grades 2 and 3 were conducted. participants in consultation with the gauteng department of education, participants were purposively selected from three schools in the inner-city area of johannesburg east, gauteng. when the study commenced, information sheets and consent forms were distributed to all the parents of grade 1 learners at each school, and those children whose parents gave table i. number and proportion of grade 1 learners speaking each language at three inner-city schools in johannesburg, gauteng sepedi 26 8.1% isizulu 135 42.06% sesotho 25 7.79% chinyarwanda 3 0.93% setswana 24 7.48% nyanja 5 1.56% siswati 1 0.31% swahili 2 0.62% tshivenda 8 2.49% tshona 5 1.56% xitsonga 8 2.49% french 29 9.03% afrikaans 7 2.18% portuguese 1 0.31% english 10 3.12% malawian 1 0.31% isindebele 2 0.62% cameronian 1 0.31% isixhosa 22 6.85% unknown 6 1.87% total 321 100% vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 45 semantic development informed consent for their participation were considered for inclusion in the study, provided they met the criteria for selection. the response rates at the three schools varied between 60% and 75%. the participants varied in socio-economic status, but were mainly from middleto low-income families. the lolt in all three schools was english, and the foundation phase teachers spoke english as either a first or additional language. all the learners at all three schools were eal. the sample size was initially 56 when the learners were in grade 1 but was reduced to 35 when the learners were in grade 3, as a result of attrition. the learners were selected according to the following criteria: • isizulu had to be their dominant first language, as it is the most widely spoken in south africa, and is the most commonly spoken language in gauteng (statistics south africa, 2001). furthermore, the reason for choosing learners from only one language group was to restrict the influence of the first language on the results of the test. • the learners were required to be developing typically in every respect including speech, language and hearing functioning. this was established from school records. research instrument: semantic subtest of the diagnostic evaluation of language variation: criterion-referenced (delv-cr) for the reasons outlined in the introduction, this test is considered to be an appropriate measure of academic language. the semantic subtest of the delv-cr consists of the following three components: verb and preposition contrast items the verb and preposition contrast items examine a child’s vocabulary organisation (seymour et al., 2003). the ability to organise words flexibly for efficient retrieval is a skill that develops throughout the school-age years (aitchison, 1987). the delv-cr assesses the organisation of verbs because they are less influenced by cultural variation than nouns; verb meanings are central to language development (tomasello & merriman, 1995); and verb lexicons have been shown to be vulnerable in languageimpaired children (rice & bode, 1993). items on this subtest analyse the ability to provide suitable verb contrasts at the appropriate hierarchical level. the verbs examined in the delv-cr include motion, grooming, breaking, corresponding, and dressing (seymour et al., 2003). the child is required to provide verbs to complete a sentence about a picture, for example: ‘the man isn’t walking, he’s … crawling.’ preposition organisation is also tested because there is a limited set and prepositions are less variable across different dialects of english. the preposition contrast items use the same structure as the verb contrast items. the purpose of this subtest is to examine the child’s skills in producing spatial and grammatical prepositions that are in contrast to the ones used in the prompts (seymour et al., 2003). the child is shown a picture, and is required to complete a sentence, for example: ‘she’s not looking at the radio, she’s listening … to the radio.’ quantifier items quantification is based on the connection between word meaning and logic, is found in all languages and occurs frequently in the language of mathematics. the use of quantifiers provides insight into how well the child’s developing grammar can manage the complex constructions that are used in everyday discourse (seymour et al., 2003). this sub-test has eight components, three of which analyse the child’s acquisition of the meaning of the quantifier ‘every’, as well as the understanding of the syntactic constraints that govern its production; another three items which examine the understanding that ‘every’ only affects the noun that follows it; and two which analyse the understanding of the syntactic constraints that regulate the production of ‘every’ across sentences (seymour et al., 2003). the child is asked to point to pictures being spoken about, for example: ‘every man is riding a horse.’ fast-mapping items fast mapping is a language skill found in all children regardless of cultural or linguistic background. learning verbs in particular is highly dependent on understanding grammar and interpreting sentence context. it is also a skill that is required in the classroom when acquiring new vocabulary, and is therefore important for academic success. this component of the semantic sub-test examines the ability to derive the meaning of an unfamiliar word from the context it is used in, after a few exposures. the first group of items included in this sub-test, teaches the child the task by using real verbs in the prompt. the second group of items makes use of novel verbs. for each item, the child is provided with a series of three pictured episodes, while the administrator expresses an action (e.g. real verb: ‘the boy is pouring juice. novel verb: ‘the girl is zanning the apple to the clown’). the child is then expected to respond to a number of questions about the characters and objects in the series of pictures, by pointing to one of four smaller pictures, using what he/ she understands about word order and word endings. the verbs used in this component consist of three variations: transitive verbs, transfer verbs and complements (seymour et al., 2003). procedure for data collection the participants were initially assessed at the end of grade 1 to ensure sufficient exposure to english to participate in the study. the second assessment was conducted towards the end of the grade 2 year and the third assessment at the end of the grade 3 year. participants were assessed individually on the delv-cr by the researchers who are experienced in language testing and familiar with the test procedure. this ensured a degree of reliability in the results obtained. the test was administered and scored according to the instructions in the manual. although the participants were given verbal encouragement throughout the testing they were not given any indication of the correctness of their responses so that any changes observed over the 3-year period would not be due to learned knowledge of the test items. ethical considerations the ethics committee for research on human subjects, university of the witwatersrand, approved the study (protocol no.: h080404). furthermore, approval was obtained from the gauteng department of education (gde) to conduct the research in an educational setting, and subsequently informed consent was obtained from the principals and teachers of the selected schools. because participants in the study were under the age of 18, informed consent was obtained from their parents/ legal guardians. in addition, assent was obtained from the children concerned (greig & taylor, 1999). the information sheets and consent forms included details regarding the general purpose of the study and the voluntary nature of participation in the study was emphasised. in addition, confidentiality and anonymity of responses and results was assured. reliability and validity in the context of this study, it was considered important to establish the validity of the semantic subtest of the delv-cr as a measure of academic language skills. validity may be defined as ‘the agreement between a measure and the quality it is believed to measure’ (kaplan, 1987, p. 254). a word definition task was administered to the learners in grade 3, and the results were correlated with the results obtained on the total semantics score of the delv-cr. snow (1990) states that word definitions are largely learned and practised at school and a word definition task is therefore considered to be reflective of academic language skill. specifically, the oral vocabulary subtest of the test of language development-primary (told-p) (newcomer & hammil, 1985) was administered. this subtest consists of 20 items which assess the ability to provide oral definitions for common english words. the learner is required to provide a brief explanation, a synonym or two major characteristics (e.g. function and appearance) of the word. each child’s score was calculated on the basis of the percentage of correct answers obtained. a pearson’s correlation coefficient revealed a strong positive correlation between the scores on the two tests [r=0.61 with a 46 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 semantic development t-stat (4.49) > critical value (1.6909), alpha level (0.05)], thus providing convergent evidence that the delv-cr is a valid measure of academic semantic skills (kaplan, 1987). reliability of the test results for each participant was established using a measure of inter-tester reliability. at each test session, approximately onethird of the children were assessed by two examiners at the same time, with one administering and both scoring the test. since administration and scoring of the test was conducted according to instructions in the manual, 100% agreement between testers was obtained. data analysis the performance of the learners on the five semantic subtests of the delv-cr at each data collection period was compared using descriptive measures of central tendency (mean) and variability (range) as well as inferential statistics (kaplan, 1987). specifically, the wilcoxon’s matched pairs signed rank test was used to conduct a within-group comparison of scores obtained in grades 1, 2 and 3. this non–parametric procedure was considered to be preferable to the parametric t-test because it does not make assumptions about the distribution of the data (kaplan, 1987). item analyses were conducted by calculating the proportion of participants getting each item correct on each subtest. this provided a clearer indication of strengths and weaknesses in semantic processing skills. results and discussion development of semantic processing skills the primary aim of the study was to track the semantic processing skills of eal learners over 3 years, using the semantic subtest of the delv-cr. the children from the three schools were treated as a single group since an analysis of variance (anova) revealed no significant differences between the learners from the different schools at the grade 1 level (f=1.2; p=0.31). in addition, no significant difference was found for gender (t=0.35; p=0.36), and thus the need to take gender differences into account was disregarded. fig. 1 illustrates the scores (in percentage) attained by the participants on each subtest of the semantics section of the delv-cr over 3 years. the results of the wilcoxon signed rank test to determine whether there were statistically significant improvements from grades 1 to 3, are reflected in table ii. statistically significant differences at the 1% level are indicated by an asterisk (*). the participants improved significantly from grades 1 to 2 on all measures except preposition contrasts and the fast mapping of novel verbs. they improved significantly on all measures from grades 2 to 3. these results indicate that exposure to english in an academic setting, over 3 years, does in fact result in a significant improvement in the learners’ ability to provide verb and preposition contrasts, to understand quantification, and to fast map novel and real verbs, all of which are important for academic purposes. this is a most encouraging finding, as it confirms that the eal learners were able to acquire the oral language skills required for schooling over time and that they benefited from the instruction they were receiving. however, there may still be doubt over the scholastic effects of this protracted period of development. this is an important implication of these results, and future research should investigate the literacy attainment of these learners and their ability to cope with the language demands of the curriculum beyond the third grade. the quantifier subtest seems to be an area of strength, in that the learners obtained an average of 63% in grade 1, which improved to 71% in grade 2 and 82% in grade 3. this may be because quantification is widely used in the language of mathematics, which we can assume the learners are exposed to in the numeracy learning area. this provides evidence for the benefit of explicit language instruction in the context of subject teaching (clegg, 1996). the learners also did relatively well table ii. statistical comparison of scores obtained in each grade on each subtest difference total semantics verb contrasts preposition quantifiers fast mapping: fast mapping: between score contrasts real verbs novel verbs z-value p-value z-value p-value z-value p-value z-value p-value z-value p-value z-value p-value grade 1 & 2 4.913 0.000* 5.246 0.000* 0.726 0.468 3.692 0.000* 3.692 0.000* 0.309 0.757 grade 2 & 3 30.972 0.00000* 6.583 0.00002* 4.33 0.01984* 7.388 0.00362* 5.777 0.00091* 6.888 0.00002* fig.1. mean scores (in %) obtained by the eal learners over a period of 3 years on the semantics subtest of the delv-cr. vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 47 semantic development on providing preposition contrasts, with average scores of 61% and 62% in grades 1 and 2, and an improvement to 72% in grade 3. the greatest improvement was seen in the acquisition of the ability to provide verb contrasts, where the learners improved from an average of 24% in grade 1 to 46% in grade 2 and 66% in grade 3. verb and preposition contrast items assess vocabulary organisation. this is an important skill because no matter how different children’s experiences and subsequent vocabulary are, the lexicon must be organised in a hierarchical pattern in order to efficiently retrieve words when needed (capone & mcgregor, 2005; seymour et al., 2003). furthermore, adequate semantic networks are essential to reading comprehension, writing cohesion, retention and recall (nicolosi, harryman & kresheck, 1989). the eal learners in this study have therefore made significant progress in their ability to organise their verb lexicon hierarchically, which would allow for easier retrieval and semantic networking, which in turn facilitates reading comprehension. fast mapping refers to the ability to guess the likely meaning of a new word after a few exposures (de villiers, 2004). this skill is important for vocabulary learning (seymour et al., 2003) and the ability to fast map novel words is particularly important to eal learners in the academic environment, as they are continually exposed to new words representing academic concepts. the learners’ fast-mapping skills improved significantly from an average of 46% in grade 1 to 55% in grade 2 to 68% in grade 3. individual variation in performance it should be noted that despite the generally positive results reported above, there is substantial individual variation in the scores obtained on each subtest (table iii), suggesting that there were children who did not do as well as the mean scores would suggest. table iii shows that the range of scores (lowest to highest) within each subtest remains similar across the 3 years. the total score for the semantics section of the delv-cr ranges from 24 to 41/50 in the third year of the study. paradis (2005) suggests that this wide variation in individual scores, which is more characteristic of second than first language acquisition, indicates that eal children are acquiring english at varying individual rates, despite having similar language experiences. although there are many affective, attitudinal, personality, social, situational and cognitive variables that may determine success in the language-learning situation (baker, 1993), the variable that most consistently correlates with language-learning success is aptitude (de bot et al., 2005). language-learning aptitude is distinct from general intelligence and includes intrinsic skills such as the ability to: identify and remember sounds of the language; recognise how words function grammatically in sentences (lexical organisation); induce grammatical rules from the input; and recognise and remember words and phrases (fast mapping) (carroll, 1981). de bot et al. (2005) claim that recent approaches emphasise the information processing components of aptitude such as working memory and phonological memory. phonological memory is assessed on non-word repetition tasks and is involved in the acquisition of vocabulary (hoff, 2005). french and o’brien (2008) and hummel and french (2010) have shown that phonological memory in children plays a role in learning a second language in the classroom. it is interesting that a different body of research reveals limitations in working memory capacity (leonard, 2003; maniela-arnold & evans, 2005) and poor non-word repetition (phonological memory) skills (botting & conti-ramsden, 2001; dollaghan & campbell, 1998; ellis weismer, tomblin, zhang, chynoweth & jones, 2000; tager-flusberg & cooper, 1999) in children with language impairment. further research on working memory and phonological memory in eal children may enable one to distinguish those who are merely slow to learn from those who are language-impaired, and may enable one to identify those at risk for language impairment (kohnert, windsor & yim, 2006). comparison with english first language (efl) learners although a comparison with efl learners was not an explicit aim of this study, an interesting issue arising from these findings is the relative standing of the eal participants in relation to a peer group. any comparison with the american criterion group participating in the development of the delv-cr would be problematic because of the different cultural environments and education systems in the usa and south africa, but there are limited data available on the performance of south african children on the delv-cr. one available study was conducted by alborough (2007) on 42 grades 1 and 2 efl learners in three schools in johannesburg. a comparison between the mean scores obtained by the learners in this study with those obtained by the participants in the alborough (2007) study is reflected in figure 2. the actual scores are contained in table iv. although statistical comparisons were not conducted, the graphs in figure 2 and the values in table iv suggest that the eal learners did not perform as well as the efl learners in grades 1 and 2, with large differences between them in grade 1 and slightly smaller differences between them in grade 2. however, the grade 3 eal learners were doing as well or outperforming the grade 2 efl learners on all subtests except for the verb contrast items, suggesting that they may catch up to their efl peers by grade 3. it is once again interesting to note that the eal and efl learners do not differ substantially on the preposition contrast or quantifier subtests, suggesting that where there is specific teaching of concepts in a subject area, such as mathematics, all children regardless of language background can learn the vocabulary of the curriculum. this phenomenon is discussed by clegg (1996), who points out that table iii. means and range of raw scores in each grade for each subtest mean scores range of scores (lowest to highest) subtest total possible grade 1 grade 2 grade 3 grade 1 grade 2 grade 3 verb contrasts 10 2.4 4.31 6.58 0 7 2 9 3 10 preposition contrasts 6 3.64 3.75 4.33 1 6 1 6 2 6 quantifiers 9 5.7 6.6 7.38 3 9 2 9 5 9 fast mapping: real verbs 10 3.64 4.87 5.77 1 7 1 8 4 8 fast mapping: novel verbs 15 4.44 4.67 6.88 1 9 1 11 2 11 total 50 19.69 24.2 30.97 12 32 13 36 24 41 table iv. comparison between mean scores (in %) attained by eal and efl learners on each subtest verb contrast preposition contrast quantifiers fast mapping: fast mapping: total score real verbs novel verbs grade 1 eal 24% 60% 63.33% 36% 29.33% 39.4% grade 1 efl 57% 68.33% 72.22% 53% 41.33% 55.4% grade 2 efl 71% 66.67% 72.22% 57% 41.33% 59.2% grade 2 eal 45% 75% 42.22% 66% 32.67% 48.6% grade 3 eal 65.8% 72.22% 82% 57.7% 45.87% 62% 48 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 semantic development one of the benefits of instruction in an additional language is that there is concurrent learning of curriculum content and the language which is the vehicle for this. this is in contrast to the teaching of a language as a subject. item analysis the results on individual items of the semantics subtests are examined in greater detail in table v, which displays the proportion (in percentage) of participants getting each item correct on each subtest. the results were compared across the 3 years, in order to provide a clearer indication of areas of strength and weakness. verb and preposition contrasts the results on the verb contrast subtest in the first 2 years indicate that the eal children experienced difficulty in providing two different verbs for the same pictured scene. the percentage of correct responses for contrast 2 (e.g. ‘the man isn’t entering the building, he’s … going out’) in both the first (16.43%) and second (32.89%) year of exposure to english was significantly lower than that obtained for contrast 1 (e.g. ‘the man isn’t walking, he’s … crawling’) (31.79% and 58.22%, respectively). however, after the third year of instruction in english, the difference between contrast 1 and 2 was reduced. these results suggest that over time the eal learners increased their verb vocabulary, and were able to organise these verbs in a more structured manner in their lexicons (de villiers, 2004). the results on the preposition contrast subtest indicated that the eal learners in the study were able to manage prepositions better than verbs, although they had more difficulty with abstract, grammatical prepositions (e.g. ‘he’s not climbing in the morning, he’s climbing … at night’) than with spatial prepositions (e.g. ‘she’s not lifting the chair, she’s sitting … on the chair’). quantifiers the learners improved in their ability to understand the quantifier ‘every’ and that it only affects the noun that follows it (e.g. ‘is every dog eating a bone’) (83.33% in the third year), as well as the conditions that regulate the production of ‘every’ across sentences (e.g. ‘the boy saw every fish. he raised his eyebrows’) (90.74% in the third year). it can therefore be concluded that a good proportion of the eal children in this study know the range of structures within which the word ‘every’ applies (seymour et al., 2003). these results were consistent across all years, confirming that this area of language does not appear to be difficult for eal learners and has been successfully developed in grade 3. fast mapping of real and novel verbs the results of this subtest showed that transitive verbs (e.g. ‘the boy is pouring the juice. which one was the pourer?’) appear to pose a greater difficulty than transfer verbs (e.g. ‘the postman is handing the letter to the boy. which one got handed?’) in both the real and novel verb fastmapping tests. although the learners improved from 37.78% to 45.37% for real verbs in grades 1 and 2 and from 42.76% to 62.5% for novel verbs in grades 2 and 3, they continue to experience difficulties in this area. with reference to real transfer verbs, a significant improvement was also noted over 3 years, as learners obtained 51.19% in grade 1, followed by 74.07% and 82.40% in grade 2 and 3, respectively. interestingly, although the eal learners demonstrated an improvement in their ability to use novel transfer verbs over the 3 years, the difference in test scores was minimal (i.e. 55.36% in grade 1, 58.67% in grade 2, and 59.72% in grade 3). this minimal difference was also evident in the first 2 years with regard to complement verbs, where the test scores table v. proportion of participants (in %) getting each item correct on each subtest in each grade grade 1 grade 2 grade 3 verb contrasts contrast 1 contrast 2 contrast 1 contrast 2 contrast 1 contrast 2 motion 41.07% 12.50% 77.78% 33.33% 91.66% 72.22% grooming 44.64% 5.36% 77.78% 8.89% 94.44% 33.3% breaking 16.07% 33.92% 35.56% 51.11% 38.88% 86.11% corresponding 10.71% 10.71% 24.44% 28.89% 25% 55.55% dressing 46.43% 19.64% 75.56% 42.22% 86.11% 75% overall total 31.79% 16.43% 58.22% 32.89% 67.22% 64.44% preposition contrasts abstract/ grammatical 63.10% 55% 63.10% spatial 58.33% 74.81% 89.81% overall total 60.71% 62.22% 72.22% quantifiers meaning of ‘every’ 56.94% 61% 75% scope of ‘every’ 50% 82% 83.33% across sentence boundaries 54.76% 62% 89.91% within sentence boundaries 67.86% 81% 90.74% overall total 63.01% 71% 82.09% fast mapping: real verbs transitive 31.0% 37.78% 45.47% transfer 51.19% 74.07% 82.40% complement 46.63% 63.70% 64.81% overall total 41.97% 56.44% 64.19% fast mapping: novel verbs transitive 36.79% 42.67% 62.5% transfer 55.36% 58.67% 59.72% complement 36.79% 39.11% 50% overall total 42.98% 46.81% 57.41% semantic total score 44.75% 54.62% 67.73% fig. 2. mean scores (in %) obtained by the eal learners from grades 1 to 3 and the efl learners in grades 1 and 2 in alborough (2007). vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 49 semantic development were 36.79% (grade 1) and 39.11% (grade 2). within the third year of exposure to english, however, the difference in test scores with regard to complement verbs improved from 39.11% to 50%. in general, the eal learners performed progressively better on every verb type for both real and novel verbs, over a period of 3 years. however, the generally poor results obtained (with the exception of fast mapping of real transfer verbs), suggest that the learners experience difficulty in abstracting the meaning of verbs from the syntactic context of a sentence. ultimately, this has implications for their academic achievement, as an inability to fast map new words effectively may hinder their ability to learn academic concepts. de villiers (2004) suggests that the results from the fast-mapping items should be compared with the results from the verb-contrast items, in order to assess the ability to learn from linguistic context. this comparison indicated that the fast-mapping results in the third year of research (64.19% for real verbs, 57.41% for novel verbs) corresponded within 5% with the verb-contrast results (65.83%). these results reinforce the fact that the learners experience difficulty in acquiring english verbs merely from linguistic context. they may therefore benefit from explicit or direct teaching of english verbs and lexical organisation skills. roeper (2004) states that teachers should attempt to remove the ambiguities for eal learners by establishing linguistic contexts that support and make these skills contextually clear. vocabulary knowledge, according to adamson (1993), is the most important aspect of oral english proficiency for academic achievement. he suggests that vocabulary taught to eal learners should be explicit, and closely linked to the students’ learning needs in their subject matter classes. measures that are thought to be useful in making the classroom more accessible to these learners include common redundancy techniques such as repetition, explanation, giving examples, explicit boundary markers, visual supports, questioning and corrective feedback, and motivating learners to extend their utterances (scarcella, 2009). slts have specialised knowledge of these techniques and can be employed effectively in the classroom to assist teachers. conclusion this study has a few limitations which should be considered in the interpretation of the results. first, the investigation was limited to the assessment of semantic processing skills and excluded other equally important aspects of language processing such as pragmatics and syntax, which are also central to academic language. these processes are also assessed on the delv-cr and should be included in further research to provide a more comprehensive picture of language development in the foundation phase. second, participants were from a specific educational context in johannesburg, gauteng, where all the learners are eal. this limits generalisability of the results to other contexts in which eal and efl learners are integrated in the same classes. the influence of different educational contexts can be investigated in future studies. despite these limitations, this study has highlighted both aspects that are cause for concern and positive aspects in the development of semantic processing and vocabulary acquisition skills by foundationphase eal learners. while their significant improvement in these skills over the 3 years is positive, we do not know the consequences of such a protracted period of oral language acquisition for the development of literacy skills. this should be ascertained in future research studies. the children showing limited development over the 3 years suggest firstly, that not all eal children acquire language skills as efficiently as others and secondly, that there are children with possible language-learning disabilities whose difficulties manifest in the additional language as a lack of progress in vocabulary acquisition and organisation skills. further studies on information-processing abilities such as working memory and phonological memory may shed light on the underlying nature of these difficulties, as well as a possible aptitude for language learning among children learning through second languages in an academic context. the study confirms that there is an urgent need for establishing collaboration between teachers and slts in the education system so that language learning may be maximised. the fact that the learners showed better performance on those aspects, which we know are directly addressed through content teaching, further substantiates the basic premise that language skills can be successfully developed through explicit instruction. specifically, this study highlighted the need for instruction in vocabulary acquisition and organisation, both of which are critical for reading comprehension and therefore literacy attainment. finally, some of the eal learners in this study seem to exhibit weaknesses in verb learning, which parallel the difficulties experienced by children with language impairment (paradis, goldberg & crago, 2005). it is precisely this overlap that may lead to incorrect identification of eal learners as language-impaired, but it also raises an important theoretical question: ‘is it not a particular vulnerability in the language, i.e. the english verb system, rather than a specific deficit in the learner that results in the observed difficulties in both eal and impaired learners?’ a positive response to this question has significant implications for the definition of language impairment as a specific deficit in the language faculty (gopnik, 1990; rice, 2003; wexler, 2003). it is possible that language-learning skills are distributed on a continuum, and that those children labelled as ‘language-impaired’ are merely functioning at the lower end of the continuum. this matter warrants further investigation. however, regardless of whether the problem is due to slow eal learning or language impairment, intervention is critical since ‘language is not everything in education, but without language, everything in education is nothing’ (wolff, in alidou, boly, brock-utne, dallio, heugh & wolff, 2006, p. 9). references adamson, h. d. (1993). academic competence. theory and classroom practice: preparing eal students for content courses. new york: longman. adler, j. (2001). teaching mathematics in multilingual classrooms. dordrecht: kluwer academic publishers. aitchison, j. (1987). words in the mind: an introduction to the mental lexicon. new york: basil blackwell. alborough, k. (2007). the application of the diagnostic evaluation of language variation (delv) to monolingual english speaking grade ones and twos in south africa. unpublished research report, submitted in partial fulfilment of the requirements for the degree: b.a. (speech and hearing therapy), university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. alexander, n. (2010). schooling in and for the new south africa. focus, 56, 7-13. alidou, h., boly, a., brock-utne, b., diallo, y. s., heugh, k., & wolff, h. e. (2006). optimizing learning and education in africa – the language factor. a stock-taking research on mother tongue and bilingual education in sub-saharan africa. paris: association for the development of education in africa (adea). baker, c. (1993). foundation of bilingual education and bilingualism. clevedon: multilingual matters. bashir, a. s., conte, b. m., & heerde, s. m. (1998). language and school success: collaborative challenges and choices. in d. d. merritt & b. culatta (eds.), language intervention in the classroom. san diego: singular publishing group. beukes, a. m. (2008). language policy implementation in south africa: how kempton park’s great expectations are dashed in tshwane. stellenbosch papers in linguistics, 38, 1-26. botting, n., & conti-ramsden, g. (2001). non-word repetition and language development in children with specific language impairment (sli). international journal of language and communication disorders, 36(4), 421-432. braam, d. (2004). community perception of change in a school’s language policy: praesa occasional papers no. 21. cape town: project for the study of alternative education in south africa. capone, n. c., & mcgregor, k. k. (2005). the effect of semantic representation on toddlers’ word retrieval. journal of speech, language, and hearing research, 48(6), 1468-1481. carroll, j. b. (1981). twenty five years of research on foreign language aptitude. in c. diller (ed.), individual differences and universals in language learning. rowley, ma: newbury house. carstens, w. a. m. (2006). praktiese meertaligheid – feit of fiksie? journal for language teaching, 40(1), 1-19. clegg, j. (1996). mainstreaming esl. clevedon: multilingual matters. cummins, j. (2000). language, power and pedagogy: bilingual children in the crossfire. clevedon: multilingual matters. de bot, k., lowie, w., & verschoor, m., (2005). second language acquisition: an advanced resource book. london: routledge taylor & francis group. de klerk, v. (2002). language issues in our schools: whose voice counts? part 1: the parents speak. perspective in education, 20(1), 1-14. de villiers, j. g. (2004). cultural and linguistic fairness in assessment of semantics. seminars in speech and language, 25(1), 73-90. de wet, c. (2002). factors influencing the choice of english as language of learning and teaching (lolt) – a south african perspective. south african journal of education, 22(2), 119-124. 50 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 semantic development dolloghan, c., & campbell, t. (1998). nonword repetition and child language impairment. journal of speech, language and hearing research, 41, 1136-1146. ellis weismer, s., tomblin, j. b., zhang, x., chynoweth, j. g., & jones, m. (2000). nonword repetition performance in school-age children with and without language impairment. journal of speech, language and hearing research, 43, 865-878. french, l. m., & o’brien, i. (2008). phonological memory and children’s second language grammar learning. applied psycholinguistics, 29(3), 463-508. genesee, f., paradis, j., & crago, m. b. (2004). dual language development and disorders: a handbook on bilingualism and second language learning. baltimore: paul h. brookes. gopnik, m. (1990). feature blindness: a case study. language acquisition, 1, 139-164. granville, s., janks, h., joseph, m., mphalele, m., ramani, e., reed, y., & watson, p. (1997). english with without guilt: a position paper on language in education policy for south africa. university of the witwatersrand: johannesburg. greig, a., & taylor, j. (1999). doing research with children. london: sage publications. gules, n. (2005). the struggle for english. sunday times, 9 january. heugh, k. (2000). the case against bilingual and multilingual education in south africa. praesa occasional papers no. 6. cape town: praesa (project for the study of alternative education in south africa). hoff, e. (2005). language development (3rd ed.). wadsworth, uk: thomson learning. howie, s. (2009). the status of reading literacy in south africa. primary school. presented at the 2nd annual thrass talk together conference, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg. hummel, k. m., & french, l. m. (2010). phonological memory and implications for the second language classroom. the canadian modern language review, 66(3), 371-390. kamwendo. g. h. (2006). no easy walk to linguistic freedom: a critique of language planning during south africa’s first decade of democracy. nordic journal of african studies, 15(1), 53-70. kaplan, r. m. (1987). basic statistics for the behavioural sciences. boston: allyn & bacon inc. kgosana, c. (2006, 19 february). moedertaal eerste? liewer engels, se sommige swart ouers. rapport, p. 17. kgosana, c. (2010, 10 february). basic education in crisis. the star. kohnert, k., windsor, j., & yim, d. (2006). do language based processing tasks separate children with primary language impairment from typical bilinguals? journal of learning disabilities research and practice, 21, 19-29. language in education policy. (1997). pretoria: south african department of national education, government notice no. 282, vol. 17997. leonard, l. b. (2003). specific language impairment: characterising the deficits. in y. levy & j. schaeffer (eds.), language competence across populations: towards a definition of specific language impairment. mahwah, nj: lawrence erlbaum associates. linan-thompson, s., & ortiz, a. a. (2009). response to intervention and english-language learners: instructional and assessment considerations. seminars in speech and language, 30, 105-120. macdonald, c. a. (1990). crossing the threshold into standard three. main report of the threshold project. pretoria: human sciences research council. maniela-arnold, e., & evans, j. l. (2005). beyond capacity limitations: determinants of word recall performance on verbal working memory span tasks in children with sli. journal of speech, language and hearing research, 48, 897-909. manyike, t. v. (2007). the acquisition of english academic language proficiency among grade 7 learners in south african schools. unpublished doctoral thesis, university of south africa. maynard, k. l., pullen, p. c., & coyne, m. d. (2010). teaching vocabulary to first-grade students through repeated shared storybook reading: a comparison of rich and basic instruction to incidental exposure. literacy research and instruction, 49(3), 209-242. mclaughlin, s. (1998). introduction to language development. san diego: singular publishing. motshekga, a. (2010, 6 july). statement by minister of basic education, angie motshekga, on the progress of the review of the national curriculum statement. retrieved 12 august 2010 from http//:www.education.gov.za/statement mroz, m. (2006) teaching in the foundation stage – how current systems support teachers’ knowledge and understanding of children’s speech and language. international journal of early years education, 14(1), 45-61. newcomer, p. l., & hammill, d. d. (1985). test of language development. primary. texas: proed. nicolosi, l., harryman, e., & kresheck, j. (1989). terminology of communication disorders: speech, language, hearing (3rd ed). baltimore: williams & wilkins. ntshingila, f. (2006, 28 may). most speak zulu at home, english at work, says survey: warning that african languages face extinction. sunday times, p. 4. o’connor, j., & geiger, m. (2009). challenges facing primary school educators of english second (or other) language learners in the western cape. south african journal of education, 29, 253-269. owens, r. e. (2004). language disorders: a functional approach to assessment and intervention (4th ed). boston: pearson education. owens, r. e. (2008). language development: an introduction (7th ed.). boston: pearson education. pandor, n. (2005, 5 october). speaking notes, minister of education, naledi pandor, at the language policy implementation in heis conference, unisa, pretoria. retrieved 26 october 2006 from http://www.education.gov.za/dynamic.aspx?pageid=306&id=2290 paradis, j. (2005). grammatical morphology in children learning english as a second language: implications of similarities with specific language impairment. language, speech and hearing services in schools, 36(3), 172-187. paradis, j., goldberg, h., & crago, m. (2005, march). distinguishing between typically developing l2 children and children with sli: verb diversity and tense morphology over time. poster presentation at international symposium on bilingualism, barcelona, spain. pressley, m., levin, j. r., & mcdaniel, m. a. (1987). remembering versus inferring what a word means: mnemonic and contextual approaches. in m. g. mckeown & m. e. curtis (eds.), the nature of vocabulary acquisition. cambridge, ma: mit press. rice, m. k. (2003). a unified model of specific and general language delay: grammatical tense as a clinical marker of unexpected variation. in y. levy & j. schaeffer (eds.), language competence across populations: towards a definition of specific language impairment. mahwah, nj: lawrence erlbaum associates. rice, m. l., & bode, j. v. (1993). gaps in the verb lexicons of children with specific language impairment. first language, 13, 113-131. roeper, t. (2004). diagnosing language variation: underlying principles for syntactic assessment. seminars in speech and language, 25(1), 41-55. scarborough, h. s. (2001). connecting early language and literacy to later reading abilities: evidence, theory and practice. in s. neuman & d. k. dickenson (eds.), handbook for research in early literacy. new york: guilford press. scarcella, r. (2009). academic language for english language learners. retrieved from http//:www.colorincolorado.org seymour, h. n., roeper, t. w., & de villiers, j. (2003). delv diagnostic evaluation of language variation, criterion referenced: examiners manual. texas: psychological corporation, a harcourt assessment company: united states of america. snow, c. e. (1990). the development of definitional skills. journal of child language, 17, 697710. snow, c. e., tabors, p. o., & dickinson, d. k (2001). language development in the preschool years. in d. k dickinson & p. o. tabors (eds.). beginning literacy with language. baltimore: paul h. brookes. statistics south africa (2001). census 2001 key results. tager-flusberg, h., & cooper, j. (1999). present and future possibilities for defining a phenotype for specific language impairment. journal of speech, language and hearing research, 42, 1275-1278. taylor, n., muller, j., & vinjevold, p. (2003). getting schools working: research and systemic school reform in south africa. cape town: pearson education south africa. tomasello, m., & merriman, w. e. (eds.) (1995). beyond names for things: young children’s acquisition of verbs. hillsdale, nj: lawrence erlbaum associates. tyobeka, p. t. (2006). an open letter to all primary school principals. department of education: republic of south africa. uys, m., van der walt, j., van den berg, r., & botha, s. (2007). english medium of instruction: a situational analysis. south african journal of education, 27(1), 69-82. webb, v. n. (2004). african languages as media of instruction in south africa. language problems and language planning, 28(2), 147-173. westby, c. (1994). communication refinement in school age and adolescence, in o. haynes & b. shulman (eds.), communication development: foundations, processes & clinical applications (pp. 341-383). new jersey: prentice hall. wexler, k. (2003). lenneberg’s dream: learning, normal language development and specific language impairment. in y. levy & j. schaeffer (eds.), language competence across populations: towards a definition of specific language impairment. mahwah, nj: lawrence erlbaum associates. western and traditional medicine: cultural beliefs and practices o f sa muslims with regard to down syndrome 27 western and traditional medicine: cultural beliefs and practices of south african muslims with regard to down syndrome tasneem dangor* and eleanor ross# *speech pathology and a udiology, school o f hum an and c om m unity developm ent, university o f the w itwatersrand ^social w ork, school o f h um an and c om m unity d evelopm ent, u niversity o f the w itw atersrand a b s t r a c t the aim o f the study was to investigate the beliefs a n d practices o f caregivers a n d traditional healers within the south african m uslim community regarding d ow n syndrome. an exploratory-descriptive research design was utilized which incorporated individual interviews with 10 caregivers o f persons with d ow n syndrom e as w ell as 10 traditional healers fro m the south african m uslim community. common beliefs em anating fr o m both groups relating to the cause o f d ow n syndrom e included the notion tha t this condition was genetic in origin and that such children were p erceived to be gifts fr o m god. others attributed d ow n syndrom e to a p unishm ent fr o m g od or the result o f curses fr o m people. treatment included the use o f inscriptions fr o m the quraan, w ater that had been p ra y e d over and herbal medicines. some caregivers seem ed reluctant to approach western health care professionals due to negative p a s t experiences. the main reasons f o r consulting traditional healers were cultural beliefs a n d pressure fr o m fa m ily members, their holistic approach and the perso n a l nature o f their interventions. collaboration between allopathic medicine and traditional healing was advocated by alm ost all o f the traditional healers. these fin d in g s underline the need f o r culturally sensitive rehabilitation practices in speech-language pathology and audiology; and collaboration between w estern health care practitioners a n d traditional healers. key w ords: m uslim traditional healers; down syndrome; cultural beliefs intro ductio n differences in socio-cultural experiences, ethnic histories and family backgrounds are likely to influence people’s w orldviews regarding the aetiology o f illnesses and disorders, and the par­ ticular healing m ethods followed by individuals (battle, 2002). in terms o f worldviews, there has been a tendency to distinguish between two m ain types o f health conventions, the so-called m odem approach that is located within a western m edical para­ digm and the traditional approach, w hich is based on indigenous b elief systems (hall, 1994). w estern biom edical or allopathic medicine,, is rooted in anglo-saxon and judeo-christian value bases (tjale & de villiers, 2004) and initially tended to view disease as a form o f biological m alfunctioning, w ith ill health m anifesting in chemical, anatom ical or physiological changes (ross & deverell, 2004; tjale &jde villiers, 2004). h ealing was perceived as the scientific process o f treating disease through appropriate m edical, surgical j and chem ical interventions (chalmers, 1996). however, m ore recently, there have been at­ tempts by the w orld health organization (w ho, 2002) to inte­ grate the biom edical m odel with a social model to form a b i­ opsychosocial model w hich considers bodily functions and structures, activities perform ed by an individual, level o f partici­ pation in societal activities, and the influence o f personal, and environm ental factors on functioning, disability and health. a m ajor cause o f the pre-em inence o f w estern m edical practice, specifically in south a frica, was its connection with the coloni­ alist and later apartheid regimes w hich stressed the superiority o f w estern m edical practice (tjale & de villiers, 2004:2). u niversity o f the w itw atersrand e-m ail: rosse@ um thom bo.w its.ac.za private b ag 3 tel. +27 11 717-4481 po w its fax. +27 11 717-4573 2050 johannesburg . south a frica w ithin traditional m edicine, the terms diseases, disorders, disabilities and ailments are often used interchangeably and are generally seen as arising from natural, social, spiritual or psycho­ logical disturbances that create disequilibrium expressed in the form o f physical or mental ill health. traditional healing endeav­ ours to restore harm ony and equilibrium through natural, spiritual and psychological healing w hile the concepts o f curing and heal­ ing are also often used interchangeably (du plessis, 2003). how­ ever, the problem involved in distinguishing between western biom edical and traditional healing systems is that in the process they tend to becom e polarized and the one system is often viewed as superior to the other. for example, the late edw ard said in his canonical text on orientalism (1995) discussed the skewed view o f the other, including the islamic world, w hich was based on w estern cultural hegemony. despite the existence o f cultural hegem ony, in m any countries, people from all socio-econom ic and educational strata often utilize both biom edical approaches as well as traditional practices, creating a m edical syncretism that integrates both models and has im plications for treatm ent or m an­ agem ent o f disorders and com pliance with therapy (m uela, r ib­ era, m ushi & tanner, 2002). south african speech-language therapists and audiologists are expected to render culturally sensitive and appropriate ser­ vices to families from diverse cultural, linguistic, religious and ethnic groups. hence, they need to be aware o f the beliefs and practices o f these different groups in relation to health, illness and disability, and ways o f restoring well-being. one com m unity that forms an essential part o f the fabric o f south african society is the m uslim community. however, it is acknowledged that not all m uslim s form part o f a hom ogeneous community; nor do they share the same cultural ideologies in relation to health and heal­ ing, illness and disability. down syndrome is a disabling condition that affects over two m illion people worldwide (the n ational down syndrome society, 2004). in south africa, m any children with d ow n syn­ the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) mailto:rosse@umthombo.wits.ac.za 28 tasneem dangor and eleanor ross drome are currently undiagnosed. nevertheless, the condition w ould appear to be prevalent with a hospital /clinic diagnosis esti­ m ated to occur in less than 20% o f cases (christianson, 1995). however, despite the relatively high prevalence o f the condition, there would seem to be a paucity o f research focusing on the views o f mem bers o f the south african m uslim com m unity re ­ garding the causes and m anagem ent o f d own syndrom e or the healers that are com m only consulted in this regard. speech-language therapists and audiologist have an im por­ tant role to play, not only w ith respect to the cognitive, speech, language and hearing sequelae o f down syndrome, but in support­ ing people with d own syndrome and their families and caregiv­ ers, more specifically in multicultural settings. attitudes o f caregivers and healers often play a major role in deciding whether the child will go to school and be placed in a stimulating environm ent that promotes learning, either a special school or inclusive educational setting or rem ain at home. m an­ agem ent approaches are often influenced by cultural and religious beliefs and practices and also depend on socio-econom ic factors as well as access to facilities and resources, including mem bers o f the m ultidisciplinary team. a lthough caregivers and parents form an integral com ponent o f an effective intervention program m e in a natural setting, formal and more structured therapy should not be overlooked (bernstein & tiegerman, 1999). furtherm ore, therapy does n ot take place w ithin a vacuum, but occurs w ithin a social and cultural context. culturally sensitive practice is likely to make clients feel more com fortable in therapy; can potentially increase client compliance; and increase the likelihood o f successful treat­ m ent and interventions being achieved (d avis-m cfarland, 2002). culture and religion are inextricably intertw ined within the south african m uslim b e lie f system. a m uslim is one who sub­ mits to one g od and is a follower o f the teachings o f prophet m u­ ham m ad, who is regarded as the final messenger. w ithin islam, illness, disease and disability are all seen to be a llah ’s (g od’s) will, sent down by god. adherents o f islam are expected to act w ith compassion towards the poor, the sick and the disabled. m uslims also believe that the prophet m uham m ad was sent as a m ercy to mankind, given the wisdom by a llah with regard to healing. this approach to healing is know n as prophetic medicine (tibb-an-nabawi). it is not restricted to spiritual healing, but in­ stead balances the healing o f the soul and the physical being, so as to prepare man for the hereafter (jauziyah, 1999). the birth o f a child with a disability is not easily accepted w ithout feeling sorrow and having negative emotions. w hat helps people to deal with these feelings is the worldview to which one subscribes. caring for a child with a disability or disorder is seen as a form o f ibadah (worship). another w ay o f approaching this experience is as a challenge or test from god. d evout muslims believe that g od does not give them a test w ithout providing m ethods, which they can use to deal with this tragedy. adherents to islam are advised to share their tragedy w ith others, perform additional prayers, give extra charity and request others to pray for them (sakr, 1996). islam considers the world as a place in w hich difficulties and calamities are natural components. by know ing that difficulties are placed upon everybody and not ju st the individual, the feelings o f distress can be shared particularly with other fam ily mem bers (bayanzandeth, bolhari, ghasemabadi & ramasani, 1997). in m any eastern cultures, including the south african m us­ lim culture, families exist w ithin extended fam ily systems that form part o f collectivistic com m unities (tom oeda & bayles, 2002). a great deal o f respect, authority and decision-m aking is accorded to the elderly mem bers, as they are perceived to have acquired great wisdom. the illness o f one individual is usually seen as a predicam ent affecting not only the nuclear family but also m embers o f the extended family, and one is expected to re­ spect the advice given by older fam ily mem bers. d uring such times, one is also expected to make use o f the agents that god has provided. this includes a responsibility to seek adequate medical (or other) advice. in this regard, western health care professionals and/or eastern traditional healers m ay be consulted (bayanzadeth et al., 1997). there are three m ain groups o f m uslim traditional healers: firstly, there are m oulanas who are spiritual healers that occupy an essential and honoured position within the m uslim culture and are consulted by m any south african m uslim s for psychological, m edical and social problems. they are defined as pious islamic scholars who are well learned in all aspects o f the religion o f is­ lam. secondly, there are hakeem s, also know n as m uslim physi­ cians, who are also consulted by the south a frican m uslim com­ munity. their services include the providing o f ointm ents and m ixtures, which are m ade from herbs that are know n to have bene­ ficial healing properties and are designed to restore imbalances in the body humors i.e. blood, phlegm, bile and spleen. in addition, g ift healers, who are blessed with supernatural powers, also assist the south african m uslim families with healing/treatm ent o f vari­ ous illnesses and disabilities (desai, 1998). a lthough accurate figures are not available regarding the num ber o f south a frican m uslim s who consult w ith m uslim tradi­ tional healers, it is estim ated that approxim ately 8 out o f 10 black south africans consult w ith various types o f traditional healers in conjunction with or in preference to western trained medical prac­ titioners (keeton, 2004). the w orld h ealth o rganisation (who) also recognizes traditional healing as an integral part o f the pri­ m ary health care system in developing countries (w orld health organization, 1978:429). consequently, several studies have fo­ cused on traditional healers’ approaches to various disorders. for example, in terms o f a frican traditional healers, du plessis (2003) investigated their approaches to hiv/aids; de andrade & ross (2005) explored beliefs and practices in relation to hearing impair­ ment; while platzky & g irson (1993) focused on stuttering. a ccording to d agher & ross (2004) beliefs regarding the causation o f birth anom alies are not always grounded in empirical science, but are often understood from a m agico-religious or cul­ tural perspective (tjale & de villiers, 2004). for example, badat (2003) interviewed a group o f m oulanas from the g auteng mus­ lim com m unity regarding their approaches to cleft lip and palate. a com m on b e lie f was that cleft palate is g od sent and should not be questioned. participants in her study acknow ledged the exis­ tence o f various superstitious beliefs and practices in the muslim community. for instance, if a pregnant w om an handled a sharp object during the time o f an eclipse, her baby was likely to be born w ith a birth anomaly. in b adat’s (2003) study, emphasis was also placed on prayer and tarweez, which is an inscription from the m uslim holy scriptures w ritten on a piece o f cloth and usually w orn in the form o f an amulet. however, despite the relatively high prevalence o f down syndrome w orldwide, and the fact that m any speech-language therapists and audiologists render services to these individuals and their families, few, if any studies have focussed on the approaches o f south african^m uslim traditional healers and caregivers in relation to this condition. for these rea­ sons the study aim ed to investigate the beliefs and practices of caregivers and traditional healers within the south african muslin1 com m unity in g auteng regarding down syndrome. it was antifl' pated that this research w ould have im portant implications f°r d ie suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) western and traditional medicine: cultural beliefs and practices o f sa muslims with regard to down syndrome 29 cross-cultural awareness and culturally sensitive rehabilitation practices for various disciplines functioning in m ulticultural set­ tings; im proved referral systems and collaboration betw een w est­ ern trained health care professionals and traditional healers; incor­ poration o f cultural issues surrounding health and illness into the training curricula o f health care professionals; and further re ­ search. it was also felt that the study was both relevant and timely, given the recent prom ulgation by the south african governm ent o f the traditional health practitioners bill in 2004, w hich is de­ signed to incorporate traditional practitioners into the formal healthcare system and regulate their practice (keeton, 2004). m e t h o d o l o g y aim the aim o f the study was to investigate the beliefs and practices o f caregivers and traditional healers w ithin the south african m uslim com m unity regarding down syndrome. objectives objectives with respect to the caregivers were: 1. to obtain information regarding the time o f diagnosis, the person who conveyed the diagnosis, and participants’ under­ standing o f the term d ow n syndrome; 2. to probe personal and cultural beliefs regarding the aetiol­ ogy o f d own syndrome; 3. to elicit views regarding the m anagem ent o f d own syn­ drome w ith regard to the use o f traditional healing, medical approaches, speech-language therapy and audiology and other param edical interventions; 4. to ascertain whether caregivers had consulted w ith medical doctors, speech-language therapists and audiologists and other param edical professionals; and to explore their experi­ ences w ith these w estern trained professionals. objectives with respect to the traditional healers were: 1. to elicit from the traditional healers, personal and cultural beliefs about the aetiology o f down syndrome; 2. to examine the various m ethods used by the traditional heal­ ers to m anage/treat d own syndrome; 3. to probe attitudes o f traditional healers towards allopathic m edical practitioners and collaboration w ith western m edi­ cine; ; 4. to explore the views o f traditional healers regarding the rea­ sons for being approached by caregivers in relation to down syndrome. research design an exploratory-descriptive research design, incorporating a two-group, parallel study was employed. the rationale for adopt­ ing an exploratory-descriptive design was that it allowed explora­ tion o f a relatively unchartered area, while providing the opportu­ nity for obtaining a rich and detailed description o f m uslim tradi­ tional healing in relation to d own syndrom e (terreblanche & durrheim, 1999). the two group, parallel study, enabled the re ­ searchers to conduct individual interviews w ith a group o f care­ givers and a group o f traditional healers and thereafter to com pare the findings and extract differences and similarities from the data. due to tim e constraints, triangulation or the use o f m ultiple m eth­ ods (denzin & lincoln, 1998) was not undertaken and other m eth­ ods o f data collection were not selected. participants a purposive, non-probability sample o f 10 caregivers o f children with down syndrom e as w ell as 10 traditional healers was recruited from the lenasia, g auteng area. w ithin the purposive sam pling paradigm , “snow ball sam pling” was em ployed. prospec­ tive participants w ithin the m uslim com m unity were approached. t hey in turn were asked to obtain perm ission from other potential participants before giving their contact details to the researcher. a dvertisem ents were also placed in the local com m unity new spa­ per and on the islamic radio station, inviting mem bers o f the m us­ lim com m unity to volunteer for participation in the study. h ow ­ ever, it is acknow ledged that using a volunteer sample m ay have introduced sources o f bias. p articipan t inclusion criteria the participants were required to be south african m us­ lims, as they were likely to have an understanding and knowledge o f the com m unity’s cultural beliefs that influence their decisions. confirm ation o f the diagnosis o f down syndrome needed to have been m ade by a medical practitioner. the 10 caregivers needed to be direct and prim ary caregivers o f the child with d own syndrome and could be any m em ber o f the affected individual’s immediate family. t he traditional healers needed to be specifically trained or to have acquired some years’ experience in traditional healing so that they w ould be able to com m ent on the type o f traditional heal­ ing approaches adopted in relation to down syndrome. they also needed to have been consulted with respect to at least one person with d own syndrome. d escription o f participants the caregivers were all female and w ere all o f indian ex­ traction. eight o f the caregivers were m others, one was a sister and one was a grandm other to the person with down syndrome. the ages o f the caregivers ranged from 21 to 80 years. five o f the indi­ viduals w ith down syndrome were males and five w ere females and their ages ranged from one to 30 years. the traditional healers com prised five m oulanas, two hakeems, two spiritual healers and one herbalist. n ine o f the h eal­ ers were m ale and one was female. in terms o f ethnic group, eight were indian, one was black and one was o f m ixed descent. the period o f time spent practising traditional healing ranged from two to 22 years. r esearch instrum entation the study incorporated two sem i-structured interview schedules presented in the form o f individual interviews. copies o f the interview schedules for the caregivers and traditional healers are set out in a ppendices a and b respectively. several o f the questions were adapted from studies by bham & ross (2005) and badat (2003) and included both open and closed-ended items. both schedules were divided into two sections, nam ely a section on biographical information and a section on inform ation pertain­ ing to beliefs and practices in relation to down syndrome. content validity o f the interview schedules appeared to be dem onstrated as sufficient aspects covering the content o f the topic were investigated. in addition, a university researcher who was fam iliar w ith the area o f traditional healing scrutinized the inter­ view schedules. this person was o f the opinion that the schedules the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 30 tasneem dangor and eleanor ross had face validity as they appeared “on the face o f it” to measure w hat they purported to measure. research protocol pre-testing the interview schedule a fter ethics clearance was obtained from the u niversity ethics committee for r esearch on hum an participants, the inter­ view schedules were pre-tested on persons with sim ilar characteris­ tics to the target group. these persons were excluded from partici­ pation in the final study. due to difficulties experienced in recruit­ ing sufficient participants, pre-tests were conducted with only one m oulana and one caregiver. the pre-tests indicated that the inter­ view was fulfilling its purpose as the participants stated that they understood all the questions. thus no am endm ents were made. data collection follow ing the pre-test, the inform ation sheets and consent form s were sent out to the prospective participants. the researcher contacted both caregivers and traditional healers by telephone and invited them to participate in the study. individual appointments were made and thereafter, interviews were carried out with the traditional healers and caregivers. in order to com ply with m uslim traditions, the researcher who conducted the interview s attired her­ se lf in the appropriate m uslim dress for a female, w hich is a cloak and a head scarf, and was accom panied by a male figure to all in­ terviews with persons o f the opposite sex. she also used the appro­ priate greetings on arrival and on term inating the interviews. interview s with caregivers an d traditional healers: all the interviews w ith the caregivers took place in the com ­ fort o f the participants’ hom es and at times that were convenient for them. m ost o f the interviews with the traditional healers were conducted in places w here the traditional healers usually consulted with their patients. these areas were in the yards or gardens, close to the traditional healers’ hom es or in their offices. a t the begin­ ning o f the interviews, participants were shown pictures o f children with d own syndrom e so as to ensure correct recognition o f the syndrome. a lthough the original intention was to audiotape the interviews, participants tended to be suspicious o f and resistant to this procedure. hand w ritten field notes were therefore made o f all the responses provided by the participants. data collection contin­ ued until 10 caregivers and 10 traditional healers had been inter­ viewed, because at this point data saturation appeared to have been achieved. a ccording to leininger (1994 in m axwell & satake, 2006), saturation implies that the researcher has perform ed a “thick” description in an exhaustive effort to extract as much m eaning as possible from the data until no m ore can be said about the topic. d ata analysis the closed-ended items were analysed using descriptive statistics involving simple frequency counts, while semantic con­ tent analysis was applied to the open-ended questions in order to highligfit com m on them es expressed by participants. content analysis is a research m ethod for assem bling and analysing the content o f a text (terreblanche & durrheim, 1999). m orse (1994) has divided content analysis into two types, nam ely semantic con­ tent analysis (m anifest) and inferred content analysis (latent), se­ mantic sontent analysis is used to convey what the participants have said, while inferred content analysis infers or goes beyond w hat was said or written. n eum an (2003) em phasises the need to ensure the trustwor­ thiness or truth value and authenticity o f the qualitative framework (com parable to the positivist notions o f validity and reliability) by adopting the criteria developed by guba & lincoln (1989), namely credibility, transferability, dependability and confirm ability. by using semantic rather than inferred content analysis, the researcher aim ed to establish credibility (paralleling internal validity) o f the data as representing the “real w orld” as perceived by the partici­ pants. in terms o f transferability (which is com parable to the posi­ tivist construct o f external validity or generalizability), it was an­ ticipated that the information obtained from this study would be applicable to other therapy situations as well as to professionals who encounter clients from the m uslim com m unity in south af­ rica. in order to enhance dependability (the alternative to reliabil­ ity) o f data analysis, the same person conducted all the interviews and systematic steps adapted from terreblanche & durrheim (1999) were followed. these steps included: firstly, familiarization and immersion, which involved putting into sim pler terms by means o f reading through, m aking notes, draw ing diagram s and brain storming to obtain a general idea o f the findings; secondly, inducing themes, w hich im plied inferring general rules or classes from specific instances in a bottom up process; thirdly, coding, w hich encom passed the m aking o f different sections o f data as being instances o f or relevant to one or more o f the researcher’s themes; fourthly, elaboration, w hich involved synthesising infor­ m ation in a linear sequence; and fifthly, analysing data, interpre­ tation and inspection which included going back to all the above steps to make sense o f the data. in order to reduce researcher bias and establish confirm ability (or objectivity) o f the data, correspon­ dence checking advocated by pretorius & de la r ey (2004:31) was undertaken, whereby the prim ary researcher’s categorization o f them es was checked by her research supervisor for correspon­ dence. once agreem ent had been reached regarding categorization o f themes, these were quantified. resu lts and disc u ssio n part one: results from the interviews with the caregivers o rientation to the syndrom e i time o f diagnosis eight out o f the 10 caregivers stated that their children were diagnosed at birth. however, one participant stated thatithe first diagnosis was made during her pregnancy via an am niocentesis test. a nother mother reported that her son was only diagnosed about six months after birth. | persons who m ade the diagnosis the entire sample that was interviewed stated that their children were diagnosed w ith d own syndrome by either a gynae­ cologist or a paediatrician. understanding o f d own syndrom e nine o f the participants appeared to be aware o f the main features and characteristics o f children with down syndrome. re­ sponses included: ‘genetic disability with one less chromosome, m ongolian; l ow ears with w eak m uscle tone; d ry skin a n d prone to upper respiratory tract infections; f loppy child with stum p f i n ­ gers and two segm ents on the baby fin g er; two years slow er than norm al children. som e have a leaking heart a n d som e are mentally reta rd ed ’. however, a m other o f a four-year-old child with down die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) western and traditional medicine: cultural beliefs and practices o f sa muslims with regard to down syndrome 31 syndrome adm itted that she did n ot understand the m eaning o f the term down syndrome. genetic counselling seven o f the 10 participants inform ed the researcher that they had not been for genetic counselling. one m other gave the following reason for refusing to go for genetic counselling: ‘as a m uslim i had a child a n d was never g oing to abort, thus i fo u n d it meaningless. a fter m y d ow n syndrom e son i had two p erfectly norm al twins. ’ in contrast, one m other found that the genetic coun­ selling was useful. "my fa th e r fin a lly accepted m y son, as m y f a ­ ther was very sensitive a n d defensive a n d sa id i f anyone sa w my son they w ould laugh. ’ beliefs o f caregivers regarding the cause o f down syndrom e p ersonal beliefs regarding the aetiology o f d ow n syndrom e it should be noted that the participants tended to regard terms such as “heal” and “cure” as synonyms, w hile concepts such as “illness, disorder, disease, ailm ent and condition” were used interchangeably. a com m on b e lie f m entioned by five o f the participants was that all illnesses and birth conditions were due to g o d ’s will. this belief was sim ilar to findings by b ham & ross (2005) that m any o f the m uslim participants w hom they interviewed felt that strokes were due to g od’s will. two participants were convinced that the child was a gift from god and one must w illingly accept it and not question, ‘ why me g o d ?' o ne m other stated: ‘this is a heaven special child and only fo rtu n a te peo p le g e t these children.' a further two participants attributed the cause o f the condition to genetic factors and understood the scenario o f trisom y chrom osom es. inter-m arriage was also related to genetic factors, as one o f the participants felt that if m arriages occurred between husbands and wives who were too closely related, the risk o f having a child with down syndrome was high. one participant felt that the age o f the m other or father was one o f the causes o f down syndrome. this idea is supported in the research literature as the maternal age related risk for down syndrome is low er at age 20 (one in 1734 births), but higher at age 35 (one in 386 births) (h arperv 1998). , i c ultural beliefs regarding the aetiology o f d ow n syndrom e in addition to personal beliefs regarding the aetiology o f down syndrom e, caregivers were also asked i f they were aware o f the existence o f any cultural beliefs in their com m unities relating to this condition. four o f the participants explained that according to their culture, having a child with a disability, such as d own syn­ drome, was regarded as a punishm ent from god. punishm ent was directed to the m other, who was perceived to have committed wrong deeds in her past life. o ne caregiver em phasised that these were cultural beliefs, not islamic beliefs. furtherm ore, cultural beliefs assum ed by the com m unity were found to be closely linked to beliefs held by m any o f the participants themselves. this find­ ing was consistent with the results obtained by bham & ross (2005). several o f the m uslim caregivers and traditional healers whom they interviewed m entioned cultural beliefs regarding stroke being a form o f punishment. four o f the participants also suspected jadu (evil curses) from fam ily and friends. o ne participant added: ‘these beliefs are myths which need to be eradicated fr o m our thought patterns. in line with these findings, d agher & ross (2004) noted that the a fri­ can traditional healers in their study believed that cleft palate was caused by ancestors, spirits and witchcraft. in a sim ilar vein, three participants m entioned that m any people in their culture were u n ­ aware o f the cause o f d ow n syndrome. for example, one partici­ pant noted, ‘these ignorant p eo p le often laugh a t my son. ’ m anagem ent o f down syndrom e six o f the caregivers reported that they had consulted tradi­ tional healers regarding the m anagem ent o f their children with down syndrome. several o f the participants explained that m any o f the elderly mem bers o f their com m unities and fam ilies insisted on the use o f traditional healing. this finding was in line with the views expressed by those o f tom oeda & bayles (2002) who m ain­ tain that in collectivistic cultures such as those o f indian m uslim s in south africa, m embers o f the fam ily group tend to exert a direct influence on decisions about treatm ent options. one caregiver noted that her son was constantly being ad ­ m itted to hospital and that doctors had told her that he was not going to live long. she then approached a m oulana who gave her the tarweez and advised her to read a few verses from the q uraan in order to improve his condition. 7 was happy with the results as my s o n 's condition im proved and he also stopped crying so much ’. a nother caregiver m entioned that she took her granddaughter to a m oulana. she reported that the m oulana had read from the quraan for her granddaughter and thereafter her speech had becom e clearer. in this respect, it should be noted that traditional medicine has been shown to have several benefits, including reduced anxiety through a shared, unquestioned b e lie f in the powers o f the healer (h am m ond-tooke, 1989). one o f the participants explained that she did n ot approach traditional healers as she and her husband felt that one should ask god directly for help. she added that she and her husband read from the quraan on a daily basis and they had seen trem endous im provem ent in their daughter’s health. a nother participant shared a sim ilar view and encouraged people to read the quraan daily, as it contained shifa (cure) and a m ercy for all mankind. one participant was convinced that her daughter was a gift from god and that she had to accept her the w ay she was. a nother participant explained that her brother was physically disabled and this factor m otivated her to take care o f g od’s creatures h erself and not seek cures for disabilities. one participant inform ed the researcher that a h akeem had provided him with a herbal ointm ent to strengthen his son’s legs. “soon after treatm ent my son sta rted walking. h e also p ro vid ed m y son with a syrup f o r his constipation and this too was useful a n d i have lots o f fa ith in this h akeem a nother participant re­ ported that he approached a h akeem , not to cure his son o f down syndrom e but m erely to get her rem edy for his heart condition. she provided a diet to follow which included goat’s m ilk and som e herbal powders. the father noted that his son lost w eight and he then discontinued the diet and was not satisfied w ith the h ealer’s managem ent. a nother participant explained that her h akeem had provided her with a herbal mixture in a liquid, but her son, who was very small at time, did not drink it. thus she was not sure if it would have been effective. the caregivers’ attitudes towards western m edicine a pproaching m edical d octors] six o f the caregivers stated that they had approached m edi­ cal doctors. these participants were convinced that the doctoi the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 32 tasneem dangor and eleanor ross could assist them with this m edical condition. one o f participants explained that the doctor she approached was very encouraging and provided her with advice and m anagem ent strategies. a nother participant reported that her doctor introduced her to other m oth­ ers/caregivers o f children w ith down syndrome, which helped her deal with her feelings towards her child and contributed to more effective care and m anagem ent o f the condition. four parents stated that they had independently searched the internet and read books to assist them in the m anagem ent o f their children. t hey found the doctors to be unhelpful in terms o f giving advice, and lacking in counselling skills regarding dealing with parents’ feelings. a nother m other inform ed the researcher that her doctor had told her that her baby was going to be deaf. h owever, she explained that she and her husband used to read from the quraan and that her daughter hears perfectly well. conse­ quently, she no longer takes the doctors’ theories o r their progno­ ses in respect o f her child seriously, but perseveres with faith. a pproaching p aram edical professionals o f all the participants who were interviewed, nine stated that they had approached speech-language therapists to assist in the rem ediation process o f their children with d own syndrome. a m ong the nine children who did attend therapy, seven were cur­ rently attending, either privately or at schools. all nine caregivers stated that they had experienced success in therapy. however, one m other explained that she term inated therapy as she felt that her son had reached a plateau at the age o f 12 years. exam ples o f use­ ful aspects o f therapy included buying toys that depicted every day routines in order to facilitate basic identification; encouraging the child to vocalize and to expand his or her sentences; avoiding b aby talk; and using gestures to com plem ent verbal input. the im pression gained was that caregivers had insight into the speech language therapy services provided for their children and had im­ plem ented the advice o f speech-language therapists. the one par­ ticipant who did not approach a speech-language therapist noted that her daughter started talking spontaneously and she therefore did not find the need for speech-language therapy intervention. n one o f the participants m entioned using audiological services. five o f the caregivers reported that they had consulted with other param edical professionals including physiotherapists, occu­ pational therapists, paediatricians, cardiologists and counsellors. those that had approached these param edical professionals re­ ported being satisfied with their services. however, two o f the caregivers m entioned that they did n ot find the need to seek pro­ fessional help as their children were developing adequately. care­ givers articulated the view point that they felt m ore secure and re­ laxed about going to professionals who understood their culture and their use o f alternate remedies. part two: r esults from the interview s w ith the traditional healers ten traditional healers were interviewed, all o f who reported that they had treated children w ith down syndrome. beliefs regarding the cause o f dons syndrom e p articipants’ views were elicited on the causes o f the down syndrome as it was felt that the cause w ould reflect societal beliefs about the condition. five o f the participants were o f the opinion that the aetiology o f d own syndrome could be attributed to ge­ netic factors. one o f the participants added that when family mem bers inter-m arry this behaviour also causes the child to be b o m with some kind o f anomaly. five o f the participants attributed down syndrom e to god’s will. t hey noted that babies that are bom with such a disorder are all in g o d ’s (a llah’s) hands and we should avoid questioning god. furtherm ore, such an experience was considered a ‘test’ for the parents. ''keep in m ind that sickness is given to the p a tie n t as a trial f o r the p a tie n t h im se lf or h e rse lf a n d f o r their f a m i l y this finding was consistent w ith the islamic b e lie f that the rew ard in the life to com e is based on how one reacts to a ‘te st,’ nam ely how one treats a disabled child (sakr, 1996). one participant also men­ tioned in passing that abortion, am niocentesis and sterilization were contrary to the teachings o f islam. a further theme that em anated from the responses o f four o f the participants related to cultural beliefs in curses, also known as jadu, and evil eyes o r evil spirits (jinn) from other people. hall (1994) suggests that some people are believed to have native pow­ ers, which they utilize together with medicines or charm s to inflict hurt on others. c am pbell (1998) maintains that supernatural and m agico-religious b e lie f systems distinctive to each culture, are often alien to and not easily understood by allopathic practitioners. two out o f the 10 participants attributed the condition to an im balance betw een h ot and cold in the body, w hich caused the child to be b o m with down syndrome. this im balance was re­ ferred to by the hakeems as a disequilibrium in the b o d y ’s hu­ m oral system. it was believed that this im balance could have oc­ curred during the m other’s pregnancy. they believed that the prac­ tice o f looking at the symptoms in isolation, provided only short­ term relief, rather than long term healing. they thus advocated a m ulti-dim ensional approach, which took into account an under­ standing o f patients them selves, their life contexts and life styles, and finally the ways in w hich their spirit-m ind-body interacted w ith each other in an attem pt to achieve balance and healing, thereby attem pting to establish hom eostasis o f the spirit-mind body. in this w ay one could heal the w hole patient and not just alleviate the symptoms. this type o f approach appeared to be de­ rived from unani tibb or tibb, w hich is a holistic healing system based on the philosophies o f h ippocrates, the father o f western m edicine (sykiotis, kalliolias & papavassiliou, 2006), and the well know n islamic scholar, philosopher and physician ibn sina. tibb is a type o f natural m edicine which takes into account the individ­ u a l’s body, mind and soul. tibb was practised about 150 years ago and is the foundation on w hich m odem medicine is based. unani tibb is recognized in south africa, largely due to it. being cost effective and providing an effective understanding o f the aetiology o f illness. the principles o f tibb are in accordance with the q uraan and the teachings o f the prophet m uham m ed (sina, 2004). it was clear that the traditional healers who were inter­ view ed were concerned w ith the reasons w hy a particular disease has occurred and that the search for causality was perceived to be one o f their greatest assets. in contrast with the finding that the traditional healers who were interviewed, were concerned with the reasons why a particular disorder had occurred, g reen (1988) sug­ gests that western m edical practitioners tend to show m ore concern w ith control as opposed to considering the root o f the problem. the im portance o f holistic healing highlighted by the two hakeem s, is also shared by m any black south african traditional healers, who believe that i f the m ind is healed the body takes care o f itse lf (d agher & ross, 2004). this assum pti6n contrasts with that o f western medicine, w hich contends that i f the body is healed the m ind takes care o f itse lf (hall, 1994). one o f the h akeem s noted that he usually asked h im self the follow ing questions derived from selzer (2004) during his consul­ tations, namely: ’1) what does a symptom mean? 2) h ow should it die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 55, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) western and traditional medicine: cultural beliefs and practices o f sa muslims with regard to down syndrome 33 be listened to? 3) what does this p a tie n t’s sym ptom mean in this particular p a tien t as opposed to another patient? 4) what does this sym ptom tell us about the totality o f this p a tie n t fr o m the sym p to m ’s picture? 5) h ow can i as a practitioner, be o f the greatest help to this p erson in his wholeness, a n d assist his soul m ind body com plex to achieve its healing? ’ (selzer, 2004:10). one o f the participants stated that the ruh (soul) asks god to be b o m in that state, i.e. with d ow n syndrome. he added that god provides healers and parents with the know ledge to cope with a child with d own syndrome. he further noted that g od pun ! ishes the parents by giving them a child with d own syndrome, so that the parents can becom e more conscious o f god and becom e more loving. a fem ale spiritual healer attributed the aetiology o f down syndrome to a virus, and believed that som ething had gone wrong in the m other’s womb. she explained that it was a natural devel­ opment and that there was a negative influence, w hich disturbed the developm ent o f the foetus. inform ation regarding m anagem ent o f dons syndrom e three m oulanas indicated that they w ould provide the p a­ tient w ith a tarw eez (an inscription o f verses from the q uraan on a | piece o f paper). a tarweez is usually w orn around the neck or | attached to the child’s clothing. the tarw eez serves to provide the child with protection against any evil and eradicates any evil pro ! jected onto the child. one o f the moulanas noted, ‘for every ill­ ness there is a cure, and m uslim s are encouraged to believe in the unseen’. this participant also encouraged people to approach i m edical professionals for treatm ent as god has made them avail­ able in order to help people. a nother participant noted that he had used tarweez to help stabilize the child, and the child then started “thinking and talking” , i one m oulana m entioned that he gave his patients oils, w hich were either part o f a m ixture or on their own to be applied to the body and head. in addition, he provided them with five | different types o f seeds. h e encouraged them to drink alm ond milk, which he first prayed over. the m oulana also m entioned the i use o f holy w ater (w ater that he prayed over), which he provided to m ost o f his patients. a nother m ethod that he used was to advise his patients to put salt on their bodies at night, as it ‘cools the body and makes life easy ’. j the same m oulana m entioned the use o f honey in com bi ; nation w ith hot water. he em phasised that honey was a cure for m any illnesses as stated b y the prophet muhammed. j the hakeems reported that they would exam ine the patient 1 and establish which hum our was [blocked or not functioning opti­ mally. treatm ent included changing a certain aspect o f the tem 1 peram ent o f one o f the four hum ours by providing herbal m edica­ tion. this herbal m edication often consisted o f an infusion o f powders that assisted in balancing the hum ours, attem pting to harness the b o d y ’s energy to treat itself. one o f the hakeems noted that when the illness was evi­ dent from birth as in children with d own syndrome, eradicating or achieving hom eostasis o f spirit-m ind-body was impossible. in­ stead they assisted these children by providing m edication in the form o f herbs that m inim ized the degree o f the problem. he ex­ plained that he had once provided a child with d own syndrome with a herbal balm to aid his joint/w alking pains as the leg was i very cold. a nother point that he m entioned was that children with i d own syndrom e often straggled to talk as ‘the tongue was drier a n d colder than it should b e he therefore provided m edication to increase the moisture and heat |on the tongue and this process helped to increase blood flow to the tongue. he believed that this action made the tongue m ore mobile, thereby prom oting speech production. in addition, he stressed the fact that treatm ent was ‘holistically based taking into account the m o th er’s pregnancy, the child, the effect o f the condition and his environm ent’. herbal treatm ent was identified by two o f the participants. a herbalist noted that he provided a child, w ith d ow n syndrome who had a severe hearing problem, w ith a herb m ixture in the form o f porridge, which the child had to eat every m orning. h e also provided him with a mixture, which he had to take in the m orning and at night. the herbalist stated that this child used to be hospitalised every m onth, but after his treatment, doctors were am azed at his im proved health. his m ain m edicines were made from plants, herbs and pow der o f seeds and roots, juices, leaves and minerals. on probing the specific herbs that the healer pre­ scribed for children with d own syndrome, he replied that it was his secret. ham m ond-tooke (1989) suggests that m any herbalists possess know ledge o f natural substances, which have an authentic rem edial effect but are n ot always w illing to share this know ledge out o f fear that this knowledge will be appropriated b y others which highlights the need to protect the intellectual property rights o f traditional healers. one participant em phasised the fact that no m atter what approach to m anagem ent o r treatm ent a person pursued, success and recovery were all in g od’s hands. two o f the participants believed that spiritual healing was a necessary procedure that had to be im plem ented as part o f the treatm ent o f d ow n syndrome. firstly, perm ission is sought from god to w ork on the child. thereafter they scan the body from the spiritual realm and then intervene via touch therapy. m any traditional healers believed that the cause o f d own syndrom e was due to evil spirits that had possessed the m other and the child. the spiritual healing is a process that helps to dispel the evil spirits and cleanse the patient. a nother spiritual healer m entioned that she w ould m eet both the m other and child and then clear the m other o f evil spirits that she had been carrying during pregnancy. she noted that her treatment, w hich included a com bination o f touch therapy, reiki, automatic writing and m as­ sage, helped to rem ove any negativity within the child or m other and assisted with various difficulties. one participant noted that counselling o f the parents forms an integral part o f his treatment. he dem onstrated to the parents how to approach their child with love, and com forted them by assisting them to deal w ith their spiritual needs. v iew s o f traditional healers regarding the reasons for being approached by caregivers o f children w ith down syndrom e the issue o f culture and pressure from fam ily mem bers was strongly em phasized by eight participants. one healer noted that the elderly mem bers o f the m uslim com m unity tend to feel that alternative m ethods o f healing should be attempted. he ex­ plained that m any o f these elderly people have a strong b e lie f that some ailm ents are caused by unseen forces. they also believe that religion holds a cure for m any ailments, and can improve the well being o f the child. a nother participant shared a sim ilar view and stated that ‘many peo p le have fa ith in what their grandparents believed as they grew up strong a n d h e a lth y ’. this theme was articulated by four o f the healers. one o f the participants rem arked, ‘ we w ork with uncondi­ tional love. m any o f our peo p le maintain that modern doctors are g enerally in a hurry a n d they do not give enough time, care and the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 34 tasneem dangor and eleanor ross attention, in contrast to traditional h ealers’. several o f the tradi­ tional healers em phasized the im portance o f establishing a rela­ tionship of trust and unconditional positive regard with people w ho consulted them. this approach is similar to that advocated by m any western counselling professionals (e.g. manning, 2001). t he argum ent p ut forw ard was that western doctors derived most o f their answers from the patient, whereas traditional healers con­ firm ed what their patients conveyed to them. a m oulana em pha­ sised the fact that h e took the tim e to converse with children with d own syndrom e as m any m ight be m entally handicapped. 7 g reet them and hear their stories and i see a little world open, beauty and not ju s t a child with down syndrome. i m ake the child com fortable and make the child develop a liking towards me and build up his co nfidence’. another participant explained that he sat with these children, was sensitive to their needs and paid par­ ticular attention to the w ay in which he addressed them. this them e of a holistic approach was encapsulated in the responses of four participants. one o f the h akeem s noted that caregivers tended to approach him m ore often than m edical pro­ fessionals as h e provided a holistic approach to assessm ent and treatment. a spiritual healer attributed the popularity o f her treat­ m ent to her approach being a com bination o f physical and spiri­ tual dimensions. this preference for a holistic approach to treat­ m ent was consistent with results docum ented by b ham & ross (2005) and badat (2003). previous successful results with other patients was a com ­ m on them e m entioned by three participants. thus these patients usually recom m ended other persons to their traditional healers. o ne o f the spiritual healers mentioned that she was well known w ithin her com m unity and i f the illness or condition recurs she goes back to the birth o f the child to discover the original cause. 'moreover, ju s t like people have fa ith in certain doctors, they believe ou r hands are g o o d a t healing. m any peo p le perceive my approach a t a lower level and one that is m ore affordable. there has been a revolution in that many people approach traditional h ea lers’. t he herbalist stated: ‘h erbalists can help cure things, such as bone fra ctu res a n d we can also help control illnesses such as diabetes. therefore we are successful like the doctors in treating people. likewise, we are able to control a n d manage conditions such as down syn d ro m e’. three participants em phasized that when all else fails within the world o f m odem m edicine, people tend go back to their roots in order to find a cure. this finding is similar to the assertion by c am pbell (1998) that traditional healers are usually well respected, accepted and trusted by their com m unities be­ cause they are culturally and religiously congruent with their own beliefs and practices. one traditional healer expressed the view: ‘t hey com e for security; dependent on you for a cure .. .they usually com e and see us to alleviate them from guilt. they w ant some kind of rein­ forcem ent that it is not their fault’. finally, one participant stated: ‘t hey think they’ve been cursed’. the traditional healers’ attitudes towards allopathic m edical practitioners and collaboration with w estern m edicine nine out o f th e 10 traditional healers w ho were inter­ viewed reported that at som e point they had advised the parents o f children with down syndrom e to approach a m edical doctor. o ne o f the participants expressed the view that he would like to w ork with doctors and therefore advised parents to approach die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 m edical practitioners. a spiritual healer reported that she had re­ ceived a prophecy to w ork with doctors. a nother participant ar­ ticulated the opinion that if his treatm ent was unsuccessful he then referred to doctors. in line with these findings, it has been noted that some traditional healers take a keen interest in primary health care training provided by m odern form al m edicine (van w yk, van o udtshoom & gericke, 2003) furtherm ore, a m oulana adm itted that jadu was not always the cause o f the ch ild ’s condition and in these cases he would re­ fer to m edical professionals for help. another m oulana mentioned that he regularly sat with doctors and consulted with them. most o f the participants referred patients to doctors as they felt that some children needed surgery, particularly those with heart prob­ lems. in contrast, one o f th e hakeems stated that he did not refer to medical doctors as m ost o f the patients that cam e to him had often given up hope and lost faith in m edical doctors as they had not experienced success with allopathic medicine. five o f the participants had referred their patients to speech-language and hearing therapists. one participant reported that he had not referred to a speech-language therapist, because w henever a child had presented with a speech or language prob­ lem , his treatm ent had proved successful ‘and with time the child sta rted to talk in long sentences, understand better a n d his think­ ing po w er increased’. n ine o f the participants reported that they did not consult with other m edical or param edical professions. the time factor was noted to be one o f the reasons for not consulting with other w estern trained professionals. t he herbalist stated that he did not approach other professionals, as he preferred to control the child’s condition with the use o f herbs. t he entire group o f traditional healers who were inter­ viewed supported collaboration with health care professionals and expressed a keen interest in learning about m odem m edicine and the roles o f the various team mem bers involved in the rehabilita­ tion of the child with down syndrome. they also felt that there was a need for the m odem world to be acquainted with traditional healing and that western professionals should respect this form of healing. for example, a spiritual healer stated that she would do her w ork and they (m edical doctors) would do theirs and the com­ bined effect was likely to produce optim al results for the patient. o ne participant stressed the fact that islam proclaim s that we should go out and find a cure, because h e believed that for every disease there was a cure, thus alternative m ethods should be en­ couraged, including m edical doctors’ approaches to healing. a m oulana supported collaboration with western m edical profes­ sionals as he felt that a disorder can be both spiritual and medical in aetiology and hence both realm s can potentially help in differ­ ent ways. one hakeem conveyed the view that his work included providing a balance with the hum ours, to m inim ize harm emanat­ ing from them and that other professionals were needed to aid the child with down syndrom e in other avenues. for example, he stated ‘the speech-therapist will assist the child in her expertise o f language and sp e e c h ’. in fact, two o f the participants men­ tioned that at the tim e o f the study they were collaborating with western practitioners especially when surgery was required or when their m edicine was n ot healing their patients. however, one m oulana, although in favour o f collabora­ tive treatm ents, was som ew hat dubious about th e feasibility of collaboration as w estern m edicine often failed to appreciate the connection between the body and the soul. he also e x p r e s s e d deep concern regarding the negative views that he p e r c e i v e d m any m edical doctors to hold in relation to traditional healers a n d the services they offer. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) western and traditional medicine: cultural beliefs and practices of sa muslims with regard to down syndrome 35 sum m a r y of m a in fin d in g s, lim itatio ns, r ec o m m endatio ns a n d conclusio ns sum m ary o f m ain findings in summary, com m on beliefs that em anated from both the caregivers and the traditional healers with reference to the cause o f down syndrome, included the notion that this condition was genetic in origin and that such children were perceived to be a gift from god. other frequent responses attributed down syndrome to a punishm ent from god and also a result of jadu (curses from people). com m on treatm ent/m anagem ent approaches mentioned by both the caregivers and the traditional healers included the use o f tarweez and w ater that had been prayed over w hich was p ro ­ vided by m oulanas and spiritual healers, and herbal m edicines prescribed by hakeems. both groups em phasized the need to treat children with down syndrom e with patience and unconditional positive regard, and to focus on their strengths. some caregivers seemed reluctant to approach m edical doctors relative to tradi­ tional healers and this reluctance was attributed to their negative past experiences with medical practitioners. furtherm ore, nine o f the caregivers had approached speech-language and hearing thera­ pists com pared to five o f the traditional healers. both groups re­ ported m aking lim ited use o f other param edical professionals. t he main reasons given for consulting traditional healers were cultural beliefs and pressure from fam ily m em bers, their holistic view o f m anagem ent, and the personal nature o f their approaches. c ol­ laboration between m odem m edicine and traditional healing was advocated by alm ost all o f the traditional healers. h owever, these findings need to be critically evaluated. a critique o f the study revealed several lim itations. lim itations firstly, theorists such as bhopal (1997) have questioned whether research in ethnicity and health is racist, unsound, or im ­ portant science. it is the contention o f the present w riters that such research can potentially enhance awareness of the beliefs and practices"of different groups in relation to traditional healing. sec­ ondly, as the researcher who conducted the interviews was from the same religion and part o f the sam e south african com m unity as m any o f the participants, they took for granted the fact that she was acquainted with their cultural beliefs and practices, and con­ sequently failed to elaborate and provide explanations for m any o f their answers. thirdly, as some cultural beliefs were seen to be sacred or even offensive to caregivers or the children concerned, participants were initially reluctant to adm it having such beliefs for exam ple, jadu and the evil eye, and instead seemed to fam ish socially desirable responses. only once the researcher was able to establish rapport with the participants, were some o f them able to adm it that they subscribed to such beliefs. a third lim itation re­ lated to the fact that participants were unw illing to allow the re ­ searcher to tape-record the interviews. she was therefore com ­ pelled to m ake hand written notes, which occasionally tended to detract from the flow o f the interviews. fourthly, little inform a­ tion was given regarding the type o f herbs used for treatment. pre­ sumably, the traditional healers felt that the researcher m ight ex­ pose their secrets to pharm acists and other persons who might ap­ propriate their knowledge. the fifth lim itation was related to the failure to use triangulation, which would have added rigor, breadth, and depth to the investigation (denzin & lincoln, 1998). triangulation refers to the process o f “enhancing the value o f a theory by using m ultiple m ethods and perspectives to investigate the truth” (m axwell & satake, 2006:7). a further limitation re ­ lates to the lack o f generalizability o f the data. however, a counter argum ent is that the issue o f generalizability is irrelevant to re­ search o f this nature as the purpose o f the study was n ot to obtain generalizable findings but rather to elicit a rich and thick descrip­ tion o f the phenom enon under investigation. r ecom m endations d espite these lim itations inherent in the research design and methodology, im portant recom m endations can be made in respect o f culturally sensitive rehabilitation practices in speech language pathology and audiology; collaboration between western health care practitioners and traditional healers; theory and future research. c ulturally sensitive rehabilitation practices in speech-language p ath o lo g y a n d audiology a lthough the findings cannot be generalized to the entire south african m uslim community, they suggest that some m em ­ bers o f this com m unity tend to place a great deal o f em phasis on cultural and religious beliefs. it is therefore recom m ended that speech-language therapists and audiologists need to adopt cultur­ ally sensitive practices when m anaging children with d own syn­ drom e from this com m unity as cultural beliefs m ay influence how people perceive affected individuals and how they are treated or m anaged. for example, the m uslim belief that disability is from god, m ay im pact on the m anagem ent process and needs to be taken into consideration when undertaking diagnostic evaluations and planning therapy interventions with this client population. in addition, inform ation on treatm ent recom m ended by traditional healers is useful to western health care professionals, as they need to be aw are of other form s of treatm ent that parents m ay be utiliz­ ing as these interventions m ight be useful or harm ful when used in com bination with m odem medical treatm ent methods. further­ m ore, the finding, regarding the influence o f elderly and extended fam ily m em bers, has im plications for both counselling and ther­ apy in term s o f the guilt which may be felt if certain remedies are not im plem ented or traditional healers are not consulted. such findings also underscore the im portance of involving the extended fam ily in therapy and adopting fam ily-focused interventions. c ollaboration between western health care practitioners and traditional healers the fact that alm ost all o f the traditional healers who were interview ed supported collaboration with health care professionals and expressed a keen interest in learning about m odem medicine, highlights the need for collaboration between the these two sys­ tems o f medicine. however, the finding that very few o f the tradi­ tional healers m ade referrals to param edical professionals was possibly related to the fact that the participants were not know l­ edgeable about the services provided by these practitioners. t here would thus appear to be a need for these param edical profession­ als to create public awareness o f their services and the roles they can potentially play with respect to children with down syn­ drome. m oreover, approxim ately 80% o f south africans make use o f traditional healers and an estim ated 250 000 and 300 000 tradi­ tional healers are currently practising in south africa. this wide­ spread use o f traditional m edicine has to do with issues o f afforda­ bility, cultural acceptability and accessibility (du plessis, 2003). it the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 36 tasneem dangor and eleanor ross is therefore recom m ended that western health care practitioners be educated regarding the roles o f traditional healers and the m edi­ cines they use, so that there can be greater collaboration and m u­ tual respect. the south african health m inistry is currently looking for w ays to incorporate inform al medicine into the formal health sector, and the recent prom ulgation o f the traditional health prac­ titioners bill (2004) is designed to facilitate this process. theory and f uture r esearch this exploratory-descriptive study represents an effort, in some small m easure, to enhance theoretical understanding o f the south african m uslim com m unity’s m ultifaceted approach to health, illness and disability. however, while respecting these cul­ tural beliefs and practices, one cannot endorse their effectiveness without further evidence-based research. m oreover, in view o f the fact that the small sample size and the use o f snowball sampling precluded generalization o f results to the broader population o f caregivers and traditional healers, it is recom m ended that this re­ search be replicated on a larger, more representative sample. given the point raised by one o f the traditional healers regarding the ap­ parent contradiction o f such practices as abortion, sterilization and amniocentesis, w ith the teachings o f islam, future researchers need to explore the views o f traditional versus western health care pro­ fessionals regarding the ethics o f traditional healing and western health care in relation to these practices. finally, it w ould seem to be an opportune time to begin the process o f m onitoring the im ple­ m entation o f the new traditional health practitioners bill in south a frica and assessing its effectiveness in prom oting collaboration between western m edicine and traditional healing over the next few years. in conclusion, the findings that several participants attrib­ uted down syndrom e to genetic factors as well as g od’s will, and m any o f the traditional healers had referred patients to western professionals, suggests a degree o f medical syncretism whereby “biom edical know ledge transm itted in health m essages coexists, interacts and merges w ith local pre-existing ideas and lo­ gics” (m uela et al., 2002:403). m oreover, the fact that m uslim hum oral medicine is partly rooted in the writings o f hippocrates, the father o f w estern m edicine (sykiotis et al., 2006), coupled with the finding that m any o f the caregivers in the present study utilized both eastern and w estern medicine, and all the traditional healers who were interviewed were in favour o f collaboration, suggests that these two systems do not necessarily represent incom m ensur­ able paradigm s but can potentially fulfil com plem entary functions. hence, both approaches need to be taken into consideration by speech-language therapists and audiologists seeking to render cul­ turally sensitive services to clients from the south a frican m uslim references anneren, g., & pueschel, s.m. (1996). preventative medical care. in b. stratford. & p. gunn, (eds), new approaches to down syndrome. london: casse, 143-153. badat, s. (2003). the approaches o f moulanas within the south african muslim community towards cleft lip and palate. unpublished hon­ ours research report submitted for the degree b.a speech and hearing therapy. .johannesburg: university of witwatersrand. battle, d.e. (2002). communication disorders in multicultural popula­ tions. boston, massachusetts: butterworth heinemann. bayanzadeth, s.a., bolhari, j., ghasemabadi, r.n., & ramasani, (1997). mental health in the verses o f the holy quraan fo r mental health and schools staff. iran: iran university o f medical sciences. bernstein, d.k., & tiegerman. e. (1999). language and communication disorders in children. (2nd edition). new york: macmillan pub­ lishing company. bham, z., & ross, e. (2005). traditional and western medicine: cultural beliefs and practices of south african indian muslims with re­ gard to stroke. ethnicity and disease, 15, 548-554. bhopal, r. (1997). is research in ethnicity and health racist, unsound, or important science? british medical journal, 314, 1751. burack, j.a., hodapp, r.m., & zigler, e. (1998). developmental ap­ proaches to mental retardation: a short introduction. in j.a. burack., r.m. hodapp., & e. zigler (eds.). handbook o f mental retardation and development. cambridge: cambridge university press. campbell, s.s. (1998). called to heal: traditional healing meets mod­ ern medicine in southern africa today. johannesburg: zebra press. chalmers, b. (1996). western and african conceptualisations o f health. psychology o f health 1996,12: 1-10. christianson, al. (1995). clinical features o f african neonates with down syndrome. east african journal, 72(5): 306-310. dagher, d., & ross, e. (2004). approaches of south african traditional healers regarding the treatment of cleft lip and palate. the cleft palate-craniofacial journal, 41 (5), 461-469. davis-mcfarland, e. (2002). off the rack or cut to order? creating proper cultural fit. the asha leader, 7(6), 13. desai, s.a. (1998). taleemul haq. madressah dawatul haq, umzinto: dawatul haq publishers. de andrade, v.m, & ross, e. (2005). beliefs and practices of black south african traditional healers regarding hearing impairment. international journal o f audiology, 44, 489-499. denzin, n.k., & lincoln, y.s. (eds.). (1998). the landscape o f qualita­ tive research. thousand oaks: sage. du plessis, k. (2003). turning to tradition. perspective: african journal o f hiv/aids 2003, 5: 102-107. green, e.c. (1988). collaborative programs for traditional healers in primary health care and family planning in africa. african medi­ cine in the modem world, 27:117-144. guba, e. g., & lincoln, y. s. (1989). fourth generation evaluation. newbury park, ca: sage. hall, j. (1994). sangoma. new york: g.p. putman & sons. ; hammond-tooke, d. (1989). rituals and medicines: indigenous healing in south africa. johannesburg: donker. 1 harper, p.s. (1998). practical genetic counselling, (5thedition). boston, ma: butterworth heinemann. 1 jauziyah, al qaiyim. (1999). healing with the medicine o f the prophet. riyadh: darussalam. keeton, c. (2004). sangomas to join formal medical fraternity. the sun­ day times. 9 may, p. 4. manning. w.h. (2001). clinical decision-making in fluency disorders. (2nd edition.) san diego, ca: singular. maxwell, d.l. & satake, e. (2006). research and 'statistical methods in communication sciences and disorders. canada: thomson learning. morse, j.m. (1994). critical issues'in qualitative research methods. london: sage publications. die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) western and traditional medicine: cultural beliefs and practices o f sa muslims with regard to down syndrome 3 7 muela, s.h., ribera, j.m., mushi, a.k., & tanner, m. (2002). medical syncretism with reference to malaria in a tanzanian community. social science and medicine, 55 (3): 403-413. neuman, w.l. (2003). social research methods: qualitative and quanti­ tative approaches. 5th edition) boston, ma: allyn & bacon. pennington. b.f, & bennetto, l. (1988). toward a neuro-psychology o f mental retardation. in j.a.burack, r.m. hodapp, & e. zigler (eds.). handbook o f mental retardation and development. lon­ don: cambridge university press. platsky, r., & girson, j. (1993). indigenous healers and stuttering. the south african journal o f communication disorders, 40:43-48. pretorius, t., & de la rey, c. (2004). chapter 2 a brief introduction to research approaches in psychology. in l. schwartz, c. de la rey, & n. duncan (eds.). psy­ chology: an introduction. cape town: oxford university press. reed, v.a. (1994). an introduction to children with language disorders (2nd edition). new york, merrill toronto: macmillan college publishing company. ross, e., & deverell, a. (2004). psychosocial approaches to health, illness and disability: a reader fo r health care professionals. pretoria: van schaik. said, e. (1995). orientalism: western conceptions o f the orient. london: penguin books. sakr, a.h. (1996). family values in islam. canada: international islamic publishing house. seltzer,a. (2004). natural health promotes healing with harmony. muslim view,s may 2004. cape town: university of western cape. sina, i. (2004). hakims’ holistic approach to health. muslim views, cape town: university of western cape. sykiotis, g.p., kalliolias, g.d., & papavassiliou, a.g. (2006). hippo­ crates and genomic medicine. archives o f medical research, 37, 1, 181-183. terreblanche, m, & durrheim, k. (1999). research in practice: applied methods fo r the social sciences. cape town: university of cape town press. the national down syndrome society. retrieved on 5th may 2004 from htttp://www.pcsltd.com/ndss/ tjale, a., & de villiers, l. (eds.). (2004). cultural issues in health and health care: a resource fo r southern africa. cape town: juta. tomoeda, c.k., & bayles, k.a. (2002). cultivating cultural competence in the workplace, classroom and clinic. asha leader, 7 (60): 4. van wyk, b.e, van oudtshoom, b, gericke, n. (2003). medicinal plants o f south africa. pretoria: briza. world health organisation. (1978). the alma-ata conference on pri­ mary health care. who chronicle, 32: 409-430. world health organization (who). (2002). towards a common lan­ guage fo r functioning, disability and health (icf). retrieved on 12th september 2003 from http://www.who.int/icl7beginners/bg/ pdf. the south african journal o f communication disorders, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) http://www.pcsltd.com/ndss/ http://www.who.int/icl7beginners/bg/ 38 tasneem dangor and eleanor ross appendix a i n t e r v i e w s c h e d u le f o r c a r e g i v e r s section a biographical information gender of caregiver: age of individual with down syndrome: relation of caregiver to individual with down syndrome: section b information relating to down syndrome orientation to the syndrome 1. when was__________ first diagnosed with down syndrome? 2. who made the diagnosis? 3. what do you understand by the term down syndrome? 4. has anyone explained the features of this syndrome to you? please explain. 5. have you received any genetic counselling? beliefs regarding causation 6 . what do you believe is the cause of down syndrome? 7. what do people in your culture generally believe causes down syndrome? management of the syndrome 8. with regard to the person with down syndrome that you take care of, did you approach any traditional healer such as a moulana or hakeem? 9. if yes, what advice, treatment or management did he or she rec­ ommend? 10. did the advice/management that he or she recommended, meet your expectations. in other words were you satisfied? 11. if you did not approach a traditional healer, was there any reason for not approaching a traditional healer? 12. did you approach a medical doctor? 13. if so, please describe the advice / management approach that he or she recommended and your degree of satisfaction with such ad­ vice. 14. did you consult any other medical or paramedical professionals? 15. if so, please state which professionals you consulted and describe the management approaches they recommended. 16. did you approach a speech-language and hearing therapist? 17. if so, please describe the advice / management that he or she rec­ ommended and your degree of satisfaction with such advice. 18. if you did not consult with a speech-language and hearing thera­ pist, was there any reason for not consulting such a professional? 19. are there any other views or comments you would like to share with me in relation to caring for a person with down syndrome? appendix b interview schedule for traditional healers section a biographical information gender: type o f healer: number o f years practising as a healer: section b information relating to down syndrome beliefs regarding causation 1. what do you believe is the cause o f down syndrome? 2. what do people in your culture generally believe causes down syndrome? management of the syndrome 3. have you ever been consulted regarding a child with down syndrome? 4. i f yes, please can you explain the type o f advice, management or treatment that you provide for such children? 5. have you ever advised parents o f children with d own syn­ drome to approach a medical doctor? 6 . have you ever referred any o f these children to a speech language and hearing therapist? 7. have you ever consulted with any other medical or paramedi­ cal professionals with regard to children with d own syndrome? general 8 . do you feel that traditional healers and western trained profes­ sionals such as doctors and therapists can work collaboratively to treat / manage children with down syndrome? please ex­ plain. 9. what are the reasons you think parents/caregivers o f children with down syndrome consult with you instead of, or in addi­ tion to western medical practitioners? 10. d o you have any other view s or comments you would like to share with me in relation to the treatment or management o f individuals with down syndrome? 1 die suid-afrikaanse tydskrif vir kommunikasieafwykings, vol. 53, 2006 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2. ) 2 journal .of the south african logopedic society. november ^iditoriai the field of speech therapy is dynamic, and one must expect changing ideas and trends. in our last journal the theme was "stuttering" and in this issue the emphasis is on speech disorders of organic origin. marion fleming, in her article on "relationships betiveen stammering and aphasia," suggests that stuttering may have an organic basis, and this reflects the feelings of a growing number of therapists today. ' the study of the brain-injured child is another aspect which has evoked interest in recent years, and dr. jack bangs discusses this subject with emphasis on language education. katrina de hirsch and jeanette jefferson jansky also add to our knowledge of the brain-damaged child with their paper on "word deafness" finally, dr. breckivoldt's "new aspects of voice physiology" fulfils a need which we, with our limited knowledge of language, have felt for some time. the contribution is based on recent german and french literature, and makes us more aware of european fields of research. this issue therefore indicates current ideas and needs, and should, we hope, stimulate further interest in these fields. tape recorders lightweight portable models from / 1 5 / ' u f > w c r d s 3 f and 7-jspeeds. unbreakable children's records lp. record specialists. hillbrow home electric (pty.) ltd. phone 44-8843. curzon theatre buildings p.o. box 3525 kotze street, hillbrow, johannesburg r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) editorial in an age which has seen transition both swift and encompassing, it is an achievement for a publication such as this journal to reach its twentieth year. the south african logopedic society is a relatively small one; it has experienced set-backs and frustrations, but this achievement does reflect the continuing growth and interest in the field of speech therapy in south africa. those who pioneered this project in 1948 faced a more difficult task than we do today. scientific research was meagre and unsophisticated: each therapist was his own scientist. the public was uninformed, so that there was a paucity of cases; therapeutic practices and techniques were experimental and often unreliable. today, thanks to the endeavours of the past, there is a growing interest in research, a variety of cases and welldefined techniques to meet them. for some time the need has been felt for a cumulative index of the journal, and it is appropriate that it should be published in this issue. apart from its intrinsic function, perusal of the writings since 1948 indicates some interesting trends. it is evident that the chief concern of therapists in south africa has been therapy; the patient has been the central figure. that this attitude is laudable, none will deny, and it is to be hoped that nothing will diminish the concept of therapy as the major force in our work. however, the need for an empirical approach to our particular problems is becoming urgent. we need to become aware of the finer aspects of speech and language, as well as the disorders arising from these, among the polyglot groups in this country. this is only one facet of the research potential that exists in our field; it is for the speech therapist, despite inadequate financial resources, to seek the means of instigating research programmes into speech, language, communication and hearing problems which are peculiarly south african. this plea is made in the belief that research will not be done for its own sake, but will enhance the knowledge and ability of the worker in her role as therapist. a new trend observed in the field recently, and an important one, is the application of psycholinguistics to our work. we publish a study based on a system of generative grammar, which discusses the relationship between articulatory and syntactic difficulties, and suggests therapeutic procedures as well as possibilities for further investigation. another important trend revealed by examination of the cumulative index is the swift development of learning theory and its practical application which has become a dominant force in our field during the past decade. in 1957, margaret marks wrote an article entided stuttering as learned behaviour: theoretical and therapeutic implications * in this issue we have reached the stage where the same writer discusses are we good *j. s. afr. logoped. soc., 1957, 4, 10-12. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 15, nr. 1: des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) editorial 4 behaviourists? (p. 19)—a considerable step from experiment to philosophy in ten years. of the five articles published in this issue, two others discuss work directly influenced by learning theory viz: the effect of operant conditioning techniques on dysphonia in childhood, and the part operant conditioning plays in altering the speech and behaviour of autistic children. valuable therapeutic measures have emerged from both studies, as well as suggestions for research. we are fortunate in being able to publish an article by one who may be described as the doyen of speech therapists—dr. c. van riper. research will probably disclose that more has been written on stuttering than on any topic in the field—certainly this is true of the journals of the-s.a. logopedic society—with the predictable result that it is difficult to discern a clear line of thought in any aspect of the problem. in his review of the prognostic factors in stuttering, van riper has succeeded in indicating this line, to the benefit of therapists and students. it is regrettable that lack of funds should prevent the continued publication of two issues a year. in the halcyon days of cheap printing, there were years when it was possible to do this; we hope that it may be possible again in the future. we look forward to the next twenty years of publication of this journal in the same spirit that directed the. achievements of the past—a spirit compounded of curiosity, enquiry and concern which will engender the progress we hope to see in the future. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 15, no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) redaksioneel in 'η eeu waarin verandering so vinnig. en ingrypend plaasvind, is dit voorwaar 'n prestasie vir 'n tydskrif soos hierdie om sy twintigste jaar van publikasie te bereik. die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging is relatief klein en het menige terugslag en frustrasie deurleef, tog weerspieel hierdie prestasie die steeds groeiende belangstelling in spraakterapie in suid-afrika. die persone wat bemoeid was met die publikasie van die eerste uitgawe van die tydskrif in 1948, se taak was moeiliker as ons taak vandag. wetenskaplike navorsing was beperk en onwetenskaplik: elke terapeut was sy eie wetenskaplike. die publiek was oningelig, en gevolglik was daar 'n tekort aan gevalle; terapietegnieke was in 'n eksperimentele stadium en dikwels onbetroubaar. te danke aan pogings van die verlede, is daar vandag 'n groeiende belangstelling in navorsing; 'n verskeidenheid van gevalle en duidelik gedefinieerde tegnieke om daarby aan te pas. vir geruime tyd was daar 'n behoefte aan 'n samevattende indeks van die tydskrif en dit is toepaslik om dit in hierdie uitgawe te publiseer. raadpleging van geskrifte sedert 1948 toon behalwe die intrinsieke funksie daarvan 00k sommige interessante neigings. dit is voor die hand liggend dat die terapeut in suid-afrika in die eerste plek in terapie belang gestel het; die pasient was altyd die sentrale figuur. hierdie benadering was voorwaar prysenswaardig en die hoop bestaan dat terapie steeds die dryfveer in ons werk sal bly. die noodsaaklikheid van 'n empiriese benadering word egter steeds dringender. bewuswordihg van die fynere aspekte van spraak en taal, asook die belemmerings wat hieruit mag voortspruit, binne die veeltalige gemeenskapsgroepe in hierdie land, is noodsaaklik. hierdie is maar een aspek van die navorsingspotensiaal wat hierdie veld vir ons inhou; die onus rus op die spraakterapeut om, ontoereikende finansiele bates tenspyt, die middele te vind vir die loodsing van navorsingsprojekte op die gebied van spraak-, taal-, kommunikasieen gehoorprobleme wat tipies suid-afrikaans is. hierdie oproep word gemaak met die hoop dat navorsing gedoen sal word, nie vir sy eie belang nie, maar om die kennis en bekwaamheid van die werker in die rol van terapeut te bevoordeel. 'n belangrike nuwe neiging, is die toepassing van psigolinguistiek op spraakheelkunde. ons publiseer 'n studie gebaseer op 'n stelsel van generatiewe grammatika wat die verwantskap tussen artikulatoriese en sintaktiese probleme bespreek, en heelkundige prosedure asook moontlikhede vir verdere ondersoek voorstel. ondersoek van die samevattende indeks bring 'n ander belangrike rigting aan die lig, nl. die vlugge ontwikkeling van die leerteorie en die praktiese toepassing daarvan. hierdie rigting het gedurende die afgelope dekade in 'n oorheersende dryfveer, binne die gebied van spraakterapie ontwikkel. in 1957 het daar uit die pen van margaret marks 'n artikel onder die titel tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol 15, nr. : des. 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 6 redaksioneel *stuttering as learned behaviour: theoretical and therapeutic implications? verskyn. ons bereik in hierdie uitgawe die stadium waar die skryfster die onderwerp are we good behaviourists? (bl. 19) bespreek—'n aansienlike sprong van eksperiment na filosofie in tien jaar. van die vyf artikels wat in hierdie uitgawe verskyn, bespreek twee ander werke wat regstreeks deur die leerteorie be'invloed word, nl. die invloed van operante kondisioneringstegnieke op disfonie by die kind, en die rol wat operante kondisionering speel by die verandering van spraak en gedrag van outistiese kinders. albei studies het waardevolle heelkundige norme sowel as navorsingsmoontlikhede blootgele. ons is in die bevoorregte posisie om 'n artikel te kan publiseer deur die persoon wat beskryf kan word as die doyen van spraakheelkunde—dr. c. van riper. ondersoek sal aan die lig bring dat daar meer oor hakkel as oor enige ander onderwerp binne die gebied van spraakheelkunde geskryf is—dit is ongetwyfeld waar in die geval van die tydskrifte van die s.a. logopediese vereniging—met die voorspelbare verwagting dat dit moeilik is om 'n duidelike gedagtelyn met betrekking tot enige aspek van die probleem te onderskei. van riper slaag daarin om in sy oorsig: prognostic factors in stuttering, hierdie lyn aan te wys ter bevoordeling van terapeute en studente. 1 dit is betreurenswaardig dat 'n tekort aan fondse die voortgesette publikasie van twee uitgawes per jaar verhinder; ons hoop egter dat in die toekoms ons daarmee kan voortgaan. ons sien met gretigheid uit na die volgende twintig jaar van publikasie van die tydskrif en glo dat dit in dieselfde gees, wat die dryfveer was van die prestasies van die verlede, sal geskied—'n gees bestaande uit weetgierigheid, belangstelling en ondersoek wat die vooruitgang waarop ons hoop in die toekoms, sal aanhits. *tydskr. s.a. logoped. veren., 1957, 4, 10-12. journal of the south african logopedic, society, vol. 15, no. 1: december 1968 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) sajcd 86 assessment of speech in neurological disorders: development of a swahili screening test n miller, g mshana, o msuya, c dotchin, r walker, e aris   institute of health and society, speech and language sciences, university of newcastle, newcastle-tyne, uk nick miller   kilimanjaro christian medical centre, moshi, kilimanjaro region, tanzania gerry mshana oliva msuya   department of medicine, north tyneside general hospital, north shields, uk catherine dotchin richard walker   department of medicine, muhimbili university college hospital, dar es salaam, tanzania eric aris   corresponding author: n miller (nicholas.miller@ncl.ac.uk) assessments for acquired motor-speech disorders that look at movements of the articulators would appear at first glance to be universal. this may be true for the most basic non-speech aspects of movement. we argue that assessments for speech motor control must be attuned to language-specific variables to be fully valid. we describe the rationale for, and development of a motor-speech-disorder screening test for swahili speakers which includes impairment measures as well as measures of intelligibility and speech-voice naturalness. we further describe its initial validation in terms of content validity, feasibility of administration and scoring without requirements for lengthy training and technical expertise and application to groups of people with and without parkinson’s disease in tanzania. results indicate that the protocol is ready to use in so far as it is acceptable to users (clinicians, patients), is feasible to use, shows good inter-rater reliability, and is capable of differentiating performance in healthy speakers and those whose speech is disordered. we highlight needs for further development, including issues around training, development of local norms for healthy speakers and for speakers with a variety of neurological disturbances, and extension of the tool to cover culturally valid assessment of impact of communication disorders. keywords: speech disorders, assessment, swahili, neurological, parkinson’s s afr j cd 2012;59(1):27-33. doi:10.7196/sajcd.86 communication changes are a feature of many neurological conditions. yet in many african languages there is a lack of specifically designed assessments of communication. swahili is one such language. it is spoken as a first or second language by over 60 million people, predominantly in east africa. in tanzania, kenya, uganda, the comoros and the democratic republic of congo it serves as a national language. nevertheless there are no standardised assessments for motor-speech disorders. this article reports the development and initial validation of a swahili protocol to evaluate speech changes in neurological disorders. it is intended for use by health professionals involved in managing people with neurological problems. as such it reflects an example of the challenge to devise what pascoe and norman (2011) have termed contextually relevant resources. speech (taken here to include voice) disorders affect an individual’s ability to pronounce the sounds they need to say words and make themselves understood. language disorders affect the ability to understand or retrieve words and sentences, whether in spoken or written form. language and speech impairments can appear together or arise independently of each other. our focus here is on speech. speech changes may represent the first or only signs of changing neurological function (ball, willis, beukelman & pattee, 2001; harel, cannizzaro, cohen, reilly & snyder, 2004). typically though, they co-occur with a range of other motor and cognitive impairments. speech assessment is therefore important from a (differential) diagnostic perspective. clinically it can also provide sensitive, non-invasive outcome measures of decline or recovery and of the effects of drug, surgical and behavioural interventions. speech evaluation plays an important role too, since communication changes exercise a profound social and psychological influence on the person with the disorder, and their family, even when changes are not so severe as to make speech unintelligible (miller, noble, jones, allcock & burn, 2008). speech impairment can be measured in different ways. a subsystems approach (duffy, 2005; kent, 2009) singles out breathing (capacity; control over inspiration-expiration; co-ordination with vocalisation), voice (laryngeal function; voice loudness, pitch and quality), velopharyngeal efficiency (degree of hyperor hyponasality) and articulation (pronunciation of sounds and syllables) to assess underlying speech motor performance. it is also vital to assess the consequences that changes across these subsystems might have for overall intelligibility. many speech assessments exist (duffy, 2005; kent, 2009). in so far as breathing for speech, voice loudness, rate of tongue movements in producing sounds and similar measures would appear to reflect universal aspects of motor performance, it is tempting to assume that these batteries can be applied without further consideration to any language. this may be true of the most basic speed, sustainability, force and range of movement tasks divorced from meaningful words. however, as soon as one deals with variables specific to a given language and the impact of changes on intelligibility, it is imperative not to simply translate assessments from other languages but to adapt or construct them anew for the target language (pascoe & norman, 2011; wild et al., 2005). some examples illustrate this point. human spoken languages use a vast array of sounds. however, they differ in which sounds precisely are used, which sounds contrast with others to signal differences in meaning, which sound combinations are permissible, and where in a word sounds may appear – whether it can occur initially, medially and finally or only one of two of those options; in which positions in words they differ in the nature and functions of stress and intonation patterns used. so, for instance, english does not have the implosives of igbo (nigeria) and hausa (nigeria, niger and regions of west africa) or the clicks of nama (namibia, botswana). luganda (uganda) operates with 5 vowels, twi (ghana, ivory coast) with 15, english, including diphthongs, with nearer 20. hindi (india) and english both use aspirated and non-aspirated /p/ sounds. while in hindi they signal differences in meaning (/kapi/ (copy) /kaphi/ (meaningful)), in english they do not (their distribution is determined by surrounding sounds). swahili and english both have a /ŋ/ sound, but in english this cannot occur in initial position in words. both languages have /m/, /t∫/ and /t/ sounds, but while the combinations /mt∫/ (mchuzi, curry/sauce) and /mt/ (mtori, banana soup/porridge) at the start of a word are permissible in swahili, they are not in english. contrasting intonation patterns form part of the grammatical systems of languages (e.g. signalling a statement versus a question), but how these are applied across languages is not uniform. further, in most european languages changes in tone on a vowel signal predominantly affective nuances. in many other languages a system of tones operates that signifies contrasts in word and grammatical meaning, depending on whether the word is spoken with a rising, falling, high or low pitch (wong, perrachione, gunasekera & chandrasekaran, 2009). a further important observation regarding cross-language differences in speech disorders centres on cultural variables in perceptions of change. while one may be able to ascertain that a person can generate a given force in the lips when pronouncing /p/ or sustain /a:/ at 60 decibels for 10 seconds, such measures bear no direct relationship to the speaker’s and listeners’ perception of if, how and why that represents a problem or not. the acceptability of different rates of speech or loudness levels, the significance of different alterations to voice quality (breathy, creaky; harsh, soft), the tolerance for amount and degree of dysfluencies are all strongly rooted in languageand culture-specific variables (altenberg & ferrand, 2006; bebout & arthur, 1992; kita, 2009; mackey, finn & ingham, 1997; yiu, murdoch, hird, lau & ho, 2008). given that languages differ in their sound structure and use and the perception of change in neurological disorders is strongly influenced by sociolinguistic and cultural variables, it follows, as argued above, that simply translating an assessment devised around the structure and rules of one language will be invalid if applied to a structurally different one. the solution is either to adapt an extant test to the structure of the new language or create a test specifically tailored to the new language. we aimed to create a screening assessment for speech-motor dysfunction in swahili speakers covering quantification of underlying speech-motor impairment and activity limitation levels (intelligibility and speech naturalness) (kent & kent, 2000; yorkston, strand & kennedy, 1996). given the context in which the assessment is designed to be applied (by clinicians from all backgrounds, since it may well be the case that there is no trained speech-language therapist (slt) available; health services with minimum resources), the aim was to select measures that could be accomplished with minimum training in application, scoring and interpretation, without technical equipment beyond paper, pencil and (stop)watch and if possible a simple audio-recording device. in this initial development phase we also aimed to ascertain whether the assessment was suitable for purpose, i.e. to detect differences between speakers with and without a neurological illness and be sensitive to possible changes in speakers over time. methods assessment rationale impairment measures the underlying impairment to speech in neuromuscular disorders stems from alteration in the range, strength, sustainability, stability and co-ordination of movements of the muscles/movements involved in breathing, phonation, velopharyngeal function and pronunciation (duffy, 2005). assessment of these variables is typically achieved through maximum performance speech tasks (duffy, 2005; kent, kent & rosenbek, 1987) that challenge the patient to produce a sound or word as fast, loud, long, high or low as possible. we followed a subsystems approach to assessment, adapting standard recommended clinical tasks with demonstrated validity (kent, 2009) that assess breath capacity and control for speech, voice loudness, pitch and stability, and tongue and lip movement, and fulfilled the conditions required for minimal equipment and training (appendix a). the following paragraphs elucidate. prolongation of /a:/ for as long as possible gives an estimate of air reserve for speech (kent, 2009). the task can also serve as a basis for voice assessment through attention to perceived control and appropriateness of loudness and pitch, stability (tremor; inappropriate swings in pitch or loudness), and voice quality (e.g. harsh/strained (spastic) v. breathy/weak (flaccid), diplophonic (cord palsies)). these can be measured instrumentally, but for present purposes of minimal technical outlay they can be scored perceptually (naked ear) using rating scales. control over loudness, pitch level and range can be further evaluated by asking the speaker to produce /a:/ at gradually increasing and decreasing loudness and gradually rising and lowering pitch levels. speech diadochokinetic tasks (ackermann, hertrich & hehr, 1995; gadesmann & miller, 2008; ziegler, 2002) gauge tongue and lip movement parameters. the sound in the syllable is chosen to challenge a given movement, e.g. /pa/ for lips, /ta/ for tongue tip, /ka/ for tongue dorsum. one can measure time to produce 10 repetitions or number of repetitions in 5 seconds. qualitative observations record how well the individual is able to remain on target – for instance do repetitions of /ba/ drift to what is heard as /ma/ because of velopharyngeal insufficiency? does /pa/ drift to and from /ba/ from misco-ordination between oral and laryngeal gestures, or to /fa/ from decreased excursion or strength of lip movements? important information (co-ordination of movements; speech planning; apraxic difficulties v. neuromuscular, dysarthric impairment) can be gained through alternating syllable tasks (see diadochokinetic tasks above). the individual repeats sounds as fast as possible that contrast in place of articulation, e.g. /pa-ta-ka/. time to produce 5 or 10 repetitions and ability to remain fluent and on target are measured. ideally real words are used (as done in the protocol with swahili words paa, taa, kaa), (i) since this aids understanding of the task, and (ii) because they relate more closely to real speech performance (clark, 2003; kent, 2004). activity limitation impairment measures do not necessarily relate to how far changes affect communication (hartelius & miller, 2010). to gauge the impact of changes on day-to-day activity other assessments are required. for this purpose we included a diagnostic intelligibility screen and speech naturalness rating (appendices b & c). diagnostic intelligibility tests (dit) (kent, weismer, kent & rosenbek, 1989; weismer & martin, 1992) address the problem of extremely poor intraand inter-rater reliability of rating scales for assessment of intelligibility (schiavetti, 1992). in dit patients repeat a list of words and a listener (with no knowledge of what the intended words are) responds with what they believe has been said. depending on availability and/or aims of the assessment, listeners can be clinical colleagues, family members, or untrained strangers unfamiliar with the person’s speech. since scores can differ between listener groups it is essential that on retest (e.g. after therapy) the same scorers are used. by totalling words recognised and analysing the pattern of mishearings one achieves a measure of intelligibility, as well as suggestions for sound contrasts that a speaker might have difficulty signalling. dit depend on devising matched parallel lists of words that differ by one sound from each other (minimal pairs, e.g. tea-pea, pay-pie, coat-code), and reflect the sound distributions, combinations and range of sound frequencies of the language. the protocol offers four parallel lists for swahili following these principles. for administration, the examiner can either select one of the lists, or to minimise rater learning effects or retest familiarity effects where the test is frequently applied, select one word randomly from each row to arrive at varied but matched sets of 25 words. to estimate the overall impression of speech acceptability in the context of the gender, age and cultural expectations of the community the screening test employs a 1 5 naturalness/ disorderedness rating scale (1 – definitely a problem with speech; 5 – definitely no problem with speech) (appendix c). evaluation may be based on impressions from speech during general case history taking. the sentences included in appendix b provide a more controlled task for comparisons across time and persons which are attuned to syllable (articulatorily simple v. complex), word (frequency), phrase length (shorter v. longer) and grammar and associated intonation (commands v. questions v. statements) patterns that constitute prominent variables in change perception. the sentences can also provide data to supplement ratings of pitch, loudness, stability of speech/voice (see above). time to say the sentence(s) can be used as a reliable measure of speaking rate alongside previous diadochokinetic timing for maximum syllable rate. in many neurological disorders speech output is more greatly affected when a speaker has to formulate responses themselves rather than repeat or read a prepared sentence (bunton & keintz, 2008; ho, iansek, & bradshaw, 2002). accordingly, speech examinations commonly include having a person describe an everyday activity (kent, 2009). this affords a more realistic appraisal of the impact of the underlying impairment on day-to-day communication, as well as how speech production interacts with broader language and cognitive status. in the protocol here contrasting tasks offer the possibility of examining contrasts in loudness, pitch, stability, voice quality and naturalness between simple repetition tasks (saying ‘paa’, repeating single words in the intelligibility test) and the self-formulated speech while describing a common activity (appendix a, making porridge). swallowing assessment swallowing and speech disturbances are not directly related to each other, but they do frequently co-occur and management of both often falls to the same person, in westernised countries typically an slt. hence we included as part of the screen the 150 ml water swallow test (nathadwarawala, nicklin, & wiles, 1992) that has been shown to be valid and reliable at quantifying swallowing efficiency. validation procedure the speech-motor and intelligibility measures chosen for the test have proven validity as measures of speech performance (duffy, 2005; kent, 2009; kent et al., 1989; ziegler, 2002). the current protocol was also reviewed independently by six slts specialising in acquired neurological speech disorders to judge its suitability as a screen for assessing acquired motor-speech disorders. our aim was also to examine whether conducting the test was feasible in a community with minimal training (given the lack of slts and requirement to conduct brief training of other professionals); whether it was acceptable to users; and whether the newly devised materials could potentially detect differences in performance across individuals with and without a neurological disorder. to this end we piloted the tasks on a group of people with parkinson’s disease (pd) and a group matched overall for age and gender who were non-neurologically impaired. this was to establish the feasibility of the materials and tests, not specifically to examine differences between people with and without pd, which is the subject of a separate report. participants we assessed people with pd and control participants. they were recruited from a community-based prevalence survey (dotchin et al., 2008) in hai district, tanzania. participation was by voluntary informed consent following uk and tanzanian ethics committee approved procedures. results are based on 26 people with pd (7 female) and the overall matched group without pd from the same district. the people with pd were assessed before they commenced medical therapy, and 19 of them again 12 months later, after 3 months on medication. four had died and 3 were too ill for reassessment. median age of individuals without neurological illness was 76 years (interquartile range (iqr) 67 78) and for those with pd median 78 years (iqr 70 84) (p=0.10). for the latter, mean estimated duration of symptoms was 5 years (range 0.25 19). as regards overall motor status their mean unified parkinson's disease rating scale (updrs) (goetz et al., 2003) score was 50 (range 24 97). procedures a tanzanian pd nurse specialist received 3 hours’ induction and training from an experienced uk slt covering the rationale and procedures for the test, how to make the sound recordings (edirol r1 and akg c420 headmounted microphone) and scoring of items. laminated directions sheets for field use for all tasks and for live scoring were provided. people with pd were assessed before they commenced pharmacotherapy and approximately 12 months later after 3 months of levodopa treatment. speech assessment by the nurse specialist took place in the participant’s home at the same time as assessments of their motor, cognitive, mood and social status (often in the presence of other family members). recordings were downloaded to laptop computer on site and returned on compact discs for cross-checking and analysis in the uk. data processing time for sustained ‘ah’ was noted at the time of assessment. counts for number of repetitions in 5 seconds of ‘paa’ (roof in swahili), ‘kaa’ (charcoal) and ‘paa-taa (light)-kaa’ were made at the time of assessment. following standard practice speakers attempted each maximum performance task twice. the better performance was taken as their score. results for /a:/ and syllable repetitions were compared across groups and time. speech rate for the sentence ‘wale watoto wanafanya kazi kwa bidii shambani’ (those children are working hard in the field) was calculated from the acoustic waveform in syllables per second using praat (boersma & wennink, 2011). sound pressure level variability (standard deviation (sd) of mean fundamental frequency) was measured from praat based on the same sentence. to complete intelligibility test scoring, six native swahili-speaking tanzanian medical elective students studying in the uk heard recordings in random order of participants with and without pd saying words from the four parallel word lists (appendix b). recordings were played free field (dell inspirion laptop connected to fostex personal monitor 6301b loudspeaker) in a quiet clinic office with volume setting the same for all tracks. they were blind to word-list number, speaker identity and group. half the listeners heard tracks in reverse order. for each speaker they wrote down which word they believed they heard. the derived intelligibility score was the percentage of words correctly recognised across all listeners. measurement of the 150 ml swallowing test (ml per second) was calculated from volume drunk and time taken from records at the time of testing. results validity and feasibility the screening test was deemed to have sufficient face validity as independently judged by health and other workers in the community where it was to be applied. content validity was independently confirmed by review from a panel of speech-language pathologists experienced in neurological speech disorders asked to judge whether the test adequately and appropriately screened key dimensions for a speech-voice assessment in people with neurological disturbances. neither group recommended any changes to the content or delivery of the protocol. feasibility was confirmed. time to complete all sections typically took around 15 25 minutes. with the exception of one control speaker who did not understand the nature of the syllable repetition task, all participants were able to comprehend instructions and carry out the tasks correctly. the nurse specialist was able to detail performance counts/times for items requiring live scoring. ten per cent of counts and timing for ‘paa’/‘kaa’ and ‘paa-taa-kaa’ were randomly selected and calculated from the audio-recordings by an experienced slt blind to initial measurements. there was a high correlation (spearman’s r 0.96) for counts between raters with no significant difference for either control speaker recordings or people with pd. the time to repeat ‘paa-taa-kaa’ correlation of measures was similarly high (r 0.95) and there was no statistically significant difference between raters. there were some issues around audio-recording (see discussion) which impacted on the quality and completeness of some data sets. for this reason there were variable numbers of individual scores employed for the analyses that follow. group comparisons were conducted only on pairs where there were valid matched recordings available. differences between groups table 1 displays results obtained from the participants with and without pd. columns 2, 3, and 5 present the descriptive summaries for the different groups/times while columns 4 and 6 record results for statistical tests looking at possible differences between groups/times. statistically significant results are shown in bold. on the prolonged /a:/ task, single syllable repetition rates and overall speech rate people with and without pd as groups did not perform statistically significantly differently to each other. on the more taxing multisyllable alternation task (paa-taa-kaa) there was a statistically significant difference between people with pd and controls and between baseline and follow-up for people with pd. table 1. summary statistics for groups tasks and measurements pd time 1 median/mean (sd/iqr) pd time 2 median/mean (sd/iqr) pd time 1 v. 2 control median/mean (sd/iqr) pd1 v. control prolonged /a:/ (secs) 9.04 (4.47) 10.16 (4.12) z 1.06 p 0.29 9.71 (4.38) z 0.524 p 0.60 paa in 5 secs 13.64 (5.57) 12.00 (4.45) z 0.34 p 0.74 12.44 (5.54) z 0.18 p 0.86 kaa in 5 secs 14.05 (6.21) 12.67 (4.54) z 0.44 p 0.68 13.57 (4.84) z 0.03 p 0.97 time to repeat paa-taa-kaa x5 (secs) 5.61 (1.62) 4.92 (0.43) z 4.71 p <0.001 4.5 (0.48) z 4.84 p <0.001 speech rate (syllables/sec) 0.17 (0.034) na na 0.17 (0.022) z 0.82 p 0.41 loudness/sound pressure level db n=24 48.21 (4.60) na na n=24 46.0 (6.83) z 1.13 p 0.27 loudness variability sd db n=24 4.22 (1.2)     n=24 3.86 (1.51) z 0.70 p 0.48 intelligibility (% total all listeners) n=26 72 (51-78) na na n=14 75.5 (72-89) z 2.13 p 0.03 disorderedness based on sentences (1 5, 5 normal) n=18 1.75 (1-2.5) na na n=26 4 (3.5-5) p <0.007 swallowing ml/sec 6.5 (3.73-10.89) 10.19 (6.44-12.38) z 1.7 p 0.089 19.68 (15.06-26.90) z 5.15 p <0.001 pd1 = people with pd before medication; pd2 = assessment after medication commenced; sd = standard deviation; iqr = interquartile range; na = task not assessed; n = number if not 26. the effects of altered voice and articulation were also clear in the intelligibility and disorderedness ratings. people with versus without pd scored significantly differently (p=0.03) on words correctly recognised by naïve listeners. people with pd were perceived by listeners to be significantly more disordered (p=0.007) in their speech than those without, based on perceptual rating of the sentence repetitions. differences between people with and without pd were statistically significant for the water swallow test (ml/sec). there was no statistically significant change (p=0.09) in performance between baseline and follow-up for the people with pd. discussion we have developed the first preliminarily validated screening test in swahili for speech changes in neurological disorders that is not simply an unadapted translation from english and that addresses activity limitation measures (intelligibility; naturalness) as well as impairment performance. the nurse specialist was able to acquire the skills to apply and score the test after minimal training, indicated by the absence of any data loss due to misinstructions for tasks, misapplication of tasks or misscoring. the test was acceptable to participants. there were no objections to or questioning of words and tasks used. no one refused to carry it out, whether for ethical, practical or comprehension reasons. from the large variability in performance on some tasks it appears participants occasionally appeared to give suboptimal responses. it remains to be established from further observation and analysis whether this relates to cultural influences in carrying out unaccustomed testing or whether it pertains to issues around examiner training in eliciting maximum performance. a major problem encountered, that affected quality and analysis of data, concerned difficulties with audio-track labelling and with simultaneously controlling audio-recording equipment and attending to speech performance in order to deliver live scores. these point to issues in training and methods employed for detailing live performance that must be addressed in later training development. the test tasks were able to differentiate performance levels and correctly detected differences that were expected between people with and without pd (‘paa-taa-kaa’ repetitions, ackermann et al., 1995; ho, bradshaw, cunnington, phillips & iansek, 1998; ziegler, 2002), intelligibility (miller et al., 2007) and swallowing (miller et al., 2009). the fact that betweenand within-group differences on the single-syllable repetition tasks did not reach statistical significance is unsurprising, given that the nature of speech changes in pd may not be sufficiently severe to register on impairment measures. the fact that the more challenging syllable-alternating task (ho et al., 1998) did detect significant differences supports this interpretation. similarly, on the water swallow test, the time between baseline and follow-up may not have been sufficient to expect significant changes in people with pd, especially given that they received medical intervention in the interim. as pd is a progressive condition one would expect deterioration in function over 1 year but this is likely to be counter-balanced by the drug therapy they received for 3 months before the second assessment, which had a major impact on motor function in some cases (dotchin, jusabani, & walker, 2011). the protocol is ready to use. however, there are several features that ideally require further development or need local norms against which to interpret performance. the screen should also be tested on larger numbers and on other groups of people with neurological illness (e.g. stroke). monitoring the ability of a wider group of people to apply the protocol would also be helpful rather than the one trained tester employed here. next steps also include the development of culturally appropriate questionnaire measures of perceptions of change and perceived impact of speech changes. acknowledgements. this study was partly funded by a grant from the british academy uk-africa academic partnerships. references ackermann, h., hertrich, i., & hehr, t. (1995). oral diadochokinesis in neurological dysarthrias. folia phoniatrica et logopaedica, 47(1), 15-23. doi.org/10.1159/000266338 altenberg, e. p., & ferrand, c. t. (2006). perception of individuals with voice disorders by monolingual english, bilingual cantonese-english, and bilingual russian-english women. journal of speech language and hearing research, 49(4), 879-887. doi.org/10.1044/1092-4388(2006/063) ball, l. j., willis, a., beukelman, d. r., & pattee, g. l. (2001). a protocol for identification of early bulbar signs in amyotrophic lateral sclerosis. journal of neurological sciences, 191(1-2), 43-53. doi.org/10.1016/s0022-510x(01)00623-2 bebout, l., & arthur, b. (1992). cross-cultural attitudes toward speech disorders. journal of speech and hearing research, 35(1), 45-52. boersma, p., & wennink, d. (2011). doing phonetics by computer. available from http://www.fon.hum.uva.nl/praat/ 30 september 2011. bunton, k., & keintz, c. (2008). use of dual task paradigm for assessing speech intelligibility in clients with parkinson's disease. journal of medical speech-language pathology, 16(3), 141-155. clark, h. m. (2003). neuromuscular treatments for speech and swallowing: a tutorial. american journal of speech-language pathology, 12, 400-415. doi.org/10.1044/1058-0360(2003/086) dotchin, c., jusabani, a., & walker, r. (2011). three year follow up of levodopa plus carbidopa treatment in a prevalent cohort of patients with parkinson's disease in hai, tanzania. journal of neurology, 258(9), 1649-1656. doi.org/10.1007/s00415-011-5988-y dotchin, c., msuya, o., kissima, j., massawe, j., mhina, a., moshy, a., walker, r. (2008). the prevalence of parkinson's disease in rural tanzania. movement disorders, 23(11), 1567-1572. doi.org/10.1002/mds.21898 duffy, j. (2005). motor speech disorders (2nd ed.). st louis: mosby. gadesmann, m., & miller, n. (2008). reliability of speech diadochokinetic test measurement. international journal of language & communication disorders, 43(1), 41 54. doi.org/10.1080/13682820701546813 goetz, c. g., poewe, w., rascol, o., sampaio, c., stebbins, g. t., fahn, s., seidl, l. (2003). the unified parkinson's disease rating scale (updrs): status and recommendations. movement disorders, 18(7), 738-750. harel, b. t., cannizzaro, m. s., cohen, h., reilly, n., & snyder, p. j. (2004). acoustic characteristics of parkinsonian speech: a potential biomarker of early disease progression and treatment. journal of neurolinguistics, 17(6), 439-453. doi.org/10.1016/j.jneuroling.2004.06.001 hartelius, l., & miller, n. (2010). the icf framework and its relevance to the assessment of people with motor speech disorders. in l. lowit & r. kent (eds.), assessment of motor speech disorders. san diego: plural. ho, a. k., bradshaw, j. l., cunnington, r., phillips, j. g., & iansek, r. (1998). sequence heterogeneity in parkinsonian speech. brain and language, 64(1), 122-145. doi.org/10.1006/brln.1998.1959 ho, a. k., iansek, r., & bradshaw, j. l. (2002). the effect of a concurrent task on parkinsonian speech. journal of clinical and experimental neuropsychology, 24(1), 36-47. doi.org/10.1076/jcen.24.1.36.972 kent, r. (2004). the uniqueness of speech among motor systems. clinical linguistics & phonetics, 18(6-8), 495-505. doi.org/10.1080/02699200410001703600 kent, r. (2009). perceptual sensorimotor speech examination for motor speech disorders. in m. mcneil (ed.), clinical management of sensorimotor speech disorders (2nd ed., pp. 19-29). new york: thieme. kent, r., & kent, j. (2000). task-based profiles of the dysarthrias. folia phoniatrica et logopaedica, 52(1-3), 48-53. doi.org/10.1159/000021512 kent, r., kent, j., & rosenbek, j. c. (1987). maximum performance tests of speech production. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 52(4), 367-387. kent, r., weismer, g., kent, j., & rosenbek, j. (1989). toward phonetic intelligibility testing in dysarthria. journal of speech and hearing disorders, 54(4), 482-499. kita, s. (2009). cross-cultural variation of speech-accompanying gesture: a review. language and cognitive processes, 24(2), 145 167. mackey, l. s., finn, p., & ingham, r. j. (1997). effect of speech dialect on speech naturalness ratings: a systematic replication of martin, haroldson, and triden (1984). journal of speech language and hearing research, 40(2), 349-360. miller, n., allcock, l., hildreth, a. j., jones, d., noble, e., & burn, d. j. (2009). swallowing problems in parkinson disease: frequency and clinical correlates. journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, 80(9), 1047-1049. doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2008.157701 miller, n., allcock, l., jones, d., noble, e., hildreth, a. j., & burn, d. j. (2007). prevalence and pattern of perceived intelligibility changes in parkinson's disease. journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, 78(11), 1188-1190. doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.2006.110171 miller, n., noble, e., jones, d., allcock, l., & burn, d. j. (2008). how do i sound to me? perceived changes in communication in parkinson's disease. clinical rehabilitation, 22(1), 14-22. doi.org/10.1177/0269215507079096 nathadwarawala, k. m., nicklin, j., & wiles, c. m. (1992). a timed test of swallowing capacity for neurological patients. journal of neurology, neurosurgery and psychiatry, 55(9), 822-825. doi.org/10.1136/jnnp.55.9.822 pascoe, m., & norman, v. (2011). contextually relevant resources in speech-language therapy and audiology in south africa – are there any? south african journal of communication disorders, 58, 2-5. schiavetti, n. (1992). scaling procedures for the measurement of speech intelligibility. in r. kent (ed.), intelligibility in speech disorders (pp. 11-34). amsterdam: benjamins. weismer, g., & martin, r. (1992). acoustic and perceptual approaches to the study of intelligibility. in r. kent (ed.), intelligibility in speech disorders (pp. 67-118). amsterdam: benjamins. wild, d., grove, a., martin, m., eremenco, s., mcelroy, s., verjee-lorenz, a., & erikson, p. (2005). principles of good practice for the translation and cultural adaptation process for patient-reported outcomes (pro) measures: report of the ispor task force for translation and cultural adaptation. value in health, 8(2), 94-104. doi.org/10.1111/j.1524-4733.2005.04054.x wong, p. c. m., perrachione, t. k., gunasekera, g., & chandrasekaran, b. (2009). communication disorders in speakers of tone languages: etiological bases and clinical considerations. seminars in speech and language, 30(3), 162-173. doi.org/10.1055/s-0029-1225953 yiu, e. m. l., murdoch, b., hird, k., lau, p., & ho, e. m. (2008). cultural and language differences in voice quality perception: a preliminary investigation using synthesized signals. folia phoniatrica et logopaedica, 60(3), 107-119. doi.org/10.1159/000119746 yorkston, k. m., strand, e. a., & kennedy, m. r. t. (1996). comprehensibility of dysarthric speech: implications for assessment and treatment planning. american journal of speech language pathology, 5(1), 55-66. ziegler, w. (2002). task-related factors in oral motor control: speech and oral diadochokinesis in dysarthria and apraxia of speech. brain and language, 80(3), 556-575. doi.org/10.1006/brln.2001.2614 appendix a. speech and voice changes in people with parkinson’s disease © nick miller, gerry mshana, richard walker speech and intelligibility assessment protocol (swahili version available on request)   ideally these tasks should be recorded on audio or video recorder for later scoring and comparison with later assessments. for the intelligibility test it is imperative. use a new sound track for each task.   t = time on stopwatch   ddk (diadochokinetic repetition repeat ‘paa’ for 5 secs   t say ‘paa’ (roof) as many times as you can until i say stop (i.e. after 5 seconds). demonstrate with syllable /ma/ until person has understood the task. elicit twice. score: total number of repetitions of the syllable in the time on watch (as close to 5 secs as possible) repetitions in 5 secs enter time on watch (e.g. 4.94) and total repetitions (e.g. 21)   1st attempt:   2nd attempt: appendix a. speech and voice changes in people with parkinson’s disease (continued) repeat ‘kaa’ (charcoal) for 5 secs   t instructions as for 'paa'       repetitions in 5 secs   live 1st attempt   live 2nd attempt repeat ‘paa-taa-kaa' 5 times   t can you say for me ‘paa-taa-kaa’ as fast as you can until i say stop? elicit twice. score time in seconds to say 5 complete sets time to repeat 5 times: 1st attempt:   time to repeat 5 times: 2nd attempt intelligibility list ‘i have some words i would like you to repeat after me. just repeat them in your own time at your own speed’. examiner: remember not to record you saying which word it is. remember to pause recording after person has said the previous word and restart after you have said the next word but before they reply. circle which word list was used number: 1 2 3 4 words correctly recognised by: listener 1 2 3 4 5 6               mean overall score for listeners 1 6   say /ah/ as long as possible   t ‘i want you to say /ah/ for as long as you can. like this, look (demonstrate till person has understood what to do). ok, now take in as deep a breath as you can and then slowly let it out saying 'ah' as long as you can. start watch as soon as person starts ‘ah’. elicit twice. score in secs. trial 1 secs     trial 2 secs sentences for repetition person repeats sentences after examiner in their own time. same instructions for recording as for intelligibility. this time it doesn’t matter though if scorers can hear the stimulus sentences. examiner: make sure you say commands as commands, questions as questions; if person doesn’t, no need to correct them. use sentences below after ‘intelligibility’ words   describe how to carry out everyday activity please can you describe to me from start to finish how ‘mtori’ (banana porridge) is prepared?         appendix b. intelligibility test items © n miller, g mshana, r walker (swahili version available on request) aim: the patient says the following words, one at a time, in their habitual voice/rate/loudness method: either the individual reads one of the lists (can give them the list beforehand so they can check any words not familiar with), or the examiner says the word and the speaker repeats it after them. for retest purposes the same method (reading or repetition) must be employed. if using repetition the examiner should take care not to record themselves saying the stimulus word. easiest way to avoid that is to have recorder on pause (not stop) when the examiner says the word; press start and give signal for speaker to say the word; put back on pause before next word by examiner. note: examiner to say the word in natural, habitual way, making sure not to overemphasise any sounds/syllables or say the word in a deliberate fashion. say the word only once unless the person has not heard it clearly and requests a repeat. if they repeat back the wrong word leave it and go onto next item – but record in field notes that item x was given as ‘abc’. this needs to be taken into account when calculating scores. sets of words for swahili diagnostic intelligibility screening test item number list 1 list 2 list 3 list 4 1 kana kaba kala kata 2 piga pita pika piku 3 pana pande panda panga 4 baba babu bana basi 5 cheza cheka chepe chema 6 asali asili asile sifa 7 nazi kazi vazi ngazi 8 choka choma chora chota appendix b. intelligibility test items (continued) 9 njoo njaa njia njiwa 10 sikio pitio salio kilio 11 moja hoja ngoja onja 12 paka baka taka waka 13 duma dume dua dubu 14 nyani nyati nyama nyavu 15 tatu tano taka taga 16 kaa paa zaa vaa 17 waa twaa kwaa duwaa 18 ninakula tunakula wanakula watakula 19 bandua bangua pangua pungua 20 walipata watapata ulipata utapata 21 wewe hehe yeye nene 22 pima kima hima lima 23 kaja haja paja naja 24 tuma tupa tua pua 25 nawa tawa kawa sawa   sentence repetition task aim: person reads/repeats sentences below in their normal voice, loudness and rate. method: either: individual reads the sentences out loud in normal voice (give them the sentences to look through first to check they know the words). or: speaker repeats the sentences after the examiner note on protocol sheet whether read or repeated. for retest use the same method. make sure the commands are said like commands, the questions like questions. instructions: i’m going to say some sentences/i want you to read some sentences. just say the sentence in your normal voice in your normal way. sentences for people to repeat/read 1 hapana, usiende pale! 2 alisema uje au uende? 3 wale watoto wanafanya kazi kwa bidii shambani. 4 kaka ana kuku kumi na tano? 5 ameenda sokoni kuuza kahawa yake. 6 pita huku haraka! appendix c. disorderedness/naturalness rating scale ‘how sure are you that this person has a problem with their speech?’ 1 2 3 4 5 yes, definitely yes, maybe i’m not sure no, maybe not definitely not 12. fint/rg initial problems encountered in the ttpganlzation and " integration of a speech ψήijgapy depart ment in a transvaal provtncial hospital. •bx d. m. whiting. b.a. (loĝ . (adapted from a paper presented for the degree of bachelor of arts in logopedics at the university of the witwatersrand). the purpose of this paper is to present some of the problems encountered in organising a speech therapy department in a general hospital, as it is felt that the problems differ quite widely in some respects, from those encountered in an ' education department clinic. in february 19^7 a new venture was undertaken at the pretoria general hospital:to provide treatment for yet another group ' s^s® ? a n ? i c a pp e d speech, language and hearing disorders. hitherto there had been no speech therapy pro-vided at this hospital. p accommodation. the question of accommodation proved to be the first problem encountered. the department was opened several weeks later in one small room 11 ft. χ 15 ft., three walls of which were 7 ft. high beaver-board partitioning, dividing it off from n™ filtlit ih6,°ccypati°nal therapy department, another new department to be started at the same time. this was at +?f α £ hospital in what was inappropriately known as 2°m ' * s t o r e ro°m cleared for the use of these two departments with concrete floors, no running water, poor aif l?? 811(1 w l t h l i t t l e protection from the wind and rain which blew in unmercifully at times. the noise and 陣βα?ίϊ0ΐν ϊ™ t h e 0ccupational therapy side made it quite impossible to hear oneself speak at times. some of |the difficulties which arose from these conditions were:1. : the lack of waiting facilities. 2. ; the absence of facilities to separate a child from his mother during case history taking and the discussion of his problems. 3* the problem of gaining rapport with patients, children or adults, who were fully aware that every one the other side of the partition could over-hear all we were discussing. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 13. however, this was the start from which it was hoped to build up a "better department, as the work "became recognized, and the necessity to provide more'' suitable accommodation was realized. sixteen months later the department was moved to a newly opened branch of the hospital. the new accommodation consisted of two rooms and a small entrance hall, together with the use of the stoep and the passage for waiting room facilities. the smaller room was used as a testing room and office, and the larger as a treatment room. these are in pleasant surroundings and comparatively quiet. the floors are wooden, and there is hot and cold running water, good light and ventilation. (later, when a second speech therapist was appointed, another room was added to this). equipment. as speech therapy was new at this hospital, little was known of the requirements of such a department. there was no difficulty in getting initial equipment passed. but rather externally, in locating the best sources of supply of equipment other than that which was available in hospital stocks internally, in locating exactly what helpful stocks were ' carried by each specific department in the hospital. each department existed in relation to other already existing departments, there was a certain amount of overlap in the stocks carried by certain departments, and no one individual could be found who could supply all the necessary information as to what each supplied, and who had to sign the requisitions for each (some by the matron, some by the secretary and some by the superintendent himself). educational propaganda. how were we to bring to the notice of the medical staff, that this new department was now open and ready to treat patients? ί a circular was drawn up to the effect that these facilities were now available and listing, with short explanatory notes, the types of speech, language and hearing disorders which should be referred for treatment. this was sent to all honorary staff, and all wards and departments in the hospital. personal contact with physiotherapy, occupational therapy and social services departments proved very valuable in bringing the work of this new department to the notice of the medical staff. one surgeon in particular has done a great deal towards helping establish this department, not only by planning a very workable scheme of integration for his patients r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 . requiring speech therapy, "but also in stressing the importance of speech therapy for these cases to all medical students under his tuition in addition to his qualified medical assistants. the social services department and doctors serving on the panels of certain medical "benefit societies in addition to their hospital work, have been the source of patients beine referred for treatment from external sources, such as the municipal health clinics, iscor, the railways, and the pensions department. several nursery schools in pretoria have also made use of these facilities. stimulating co-operation and integration with medical staff. this has proved by far the most difficult problem that has had to be tackled, and the one in which the least results have so far been achieved* this problem has become increasingly difficult since the department was moved, as there is far less chance of personal contact and keeping track of the ever changing resident medical staff. the apparent lack of interest and co-operation from many of the medical people is probably due part to the fact that thev are very busy, in part to the tendency to be wary of anything that is new and in part to ignorance. they have very δ little knowledge of speech defects, their etiologies, and ° f ° o r r ? c t i o n > trough no fault of their own. they therefore do not realize that their assessment of the patient a? ttantn?jual γ t h e p h y 8 l c a l > intellectual and esouon" rfpffo???' m a y h a v e a n important bearing on his speech h t h * p r ^ n o s i s a n d p l a n of treatment. there seems to snctfon ^ t j « r + r n y ° f t h e m t 0 r e ^ a r d spee°h as an isolated ninetion, rather than as our primary means of communication and an integral part of the total personality. t^soiva!^0^1 1 1 w h i c h w e £ b speech .therapists, must learn fes^ir j o u r own efforts, to show the medical proxm ΐ ? 1,fr w e w a n t t 0 k n o w a n d without building able + ο exni rmη "fst ^ a r n m e d l c a l terminology, and be δ explain clearly and concisely what it is we require sρeech cnrrppt° g u ? t e 5 e n r y ' spe&king to the american speech correction association "i:t is up to you to make your medical wants known, yourselves will have to educate the physician". m e d i c a l m e n have been regarded as superior ?a v?,l e a i?t to expect this from, not only their ί ',ϊ members of the staff. this must always alissl .?i n d contacting them for information and assistance if one is to build up an attitude of co-operation. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 . a farther problem to combat is the tendency of many medicos t 0 withhold from the patient, in the case of an adult, and from the parent, in the case of a child, the true diagnosis. this has "been particularly evident in the case of dysphasias and dyslogias. they most assuredly have their reasons for this tendency, and it would "be unethical to contradict, to the patient, the diagnosis given to him "by the doctor. whereas if the patient and/or parents could acquire an understanding ' of the problem and its future implications, it would facilitate the planning of treatment and the discussion with them of the program tr "be followed. * moreover, it helps them to adjust more adequately to the problem as it stands in the present, and as it will alter in the future. there is also the need to maintain the interest of the doctors in the progress of their patients undergoing speech therapy. as compared with other treatments, this is often a long-term treatment its results "being slower and less obvious. often, at any one time, the progress in social and economic adjustment is of more importance than that of the actual improvement in the speech, as many of these problems arise from the patient sattitude and reactions to his specific speech diffic u ii y ^ jt' 1 8 e8sen"tial for us to consider each patient as an individual, interpreting his difficulties in the light of his own personality, particularly as one so often finds that doctors and staff of other departments are too "busy to consider him -in this light. integration with other departments. for the smooth running of any one department, it is essential to co-operate with other departments. therefore it is necessary to know something of the nature of their work and how they operate, and to maintain friendly though disciplined relations with the staff of each of them. this is "by no means, always easy, many departments "being cramped, understaffed and very "busy. it "becomes far easier when one realises rthat their haste and irritability is generally a reflection of their working conditions, and is not directed at one personally. this integration involves:telephone exchange, workshops, linen room, stores, dispensary, kitchen, the porters, and administration staff as well as the wards, radiography, physiotherapy, occupational therapy, and social services •departments. administration. this has two aspects. firstly that of securing the assistance and co-operation of the general administrative staff of the hospital wherever necessary, e.g. in connection with accounts, typing, signing of orders, .roneoed material, registration of r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 1 patients, and supplies. secondly, that of the administrative side of the department itself. this takes up a considerable time, and involving many items: e.g. requisitions, the checking up of " equipment ordered and not yet received, letter writing registration cards, the correlation of attendance schedules with other departments, searching for data in bedcards and files, copying of relevant data for records, supervising the follow-up and rechecking of all temporarily discharged patients, and attendance register of patients, files on each patient covering the treatment he is receiving, together with the many other items which arise from the handling of specific problems and patients. there are several other problems which could be discussed at some length, which i shall just mention here:1. development of a plan of co-operation with all other speech therapists employed by other organizations in the same area, so as to prevent overlap and at the same time provide the best treatment available for each particular patient. 2. associations and integration with civic groups, organizations and institutions in the area and further afield, whose assistance and advice might prove valuable during the treatment of any one patient. only some of the more important problems encountered in setting up a speech therapy department in a general hospital have been discussed here. many others are met in establishing such, a department but none are unsurmountable. there are frequently times when one feels inclined to let things slide because they seem too difficult jto surmount for the time being, and the results seem so slow in coming. it is at such times that it is essential for us to remember that we "belong to a profession which is still very young in this country, and which has to fight every inch of the way towards recognition. the part each and every one of us has to play towards this end is never to let circumstances overwhelm and defeat our ultimate purpose. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η ondersoek na sekere biolinguistiese verskynsels by hakkel i . c . u y s , b . a . log. (rand), m . a . (pretoria) hakkel is waarskynlik die kommunikasie-afwyking wat, op die gebied van die spraakpatologie, die meeste aan navorsing onderworpe was. veral gedurende die jare 1930 tot 1940 is verskeie studies gedoen in verband met die kenmerke van hierdie probleem. terapeute het meer bewus begin raak van die eienskappe en probleme van die hakkelaar en, afhangende van hulle persoonlike orientasie, is teoriee saamgestel wat as verwysingsraamwerk moes dien vir die verklaring van hakkelgedrag. in terme van hierdie verduidelikingsraamwerk dui die toestand vandag nog op 'n primitiewe stadium van ontwikkeling, met 'n massa teoriee en navorsingresultate wat weersprekende verslae lewer van 'n beperkte omvang van feite. die aanneemlikste verklaring hiervoor is die gebrek aan volgehoue, versigtige, gekoordineerde en sistematiese navorsing, met die doel om 'n verklaring of beskrywing van die funksionele verhouding tussen alle beskikbare feite te gee. ook wat hierdie navorsing betref, moet onthou word dat die geheel belangriker is as die som van die dele. sekere van die elemente wat alreeds ondersoek is, is die voorkoms van hakkel onder verskillende kulturele omstandighede. die resultate van hierdie ondersoeke het egter gegewens aan die lig gebring wat nog steeds onverklaarbaar voorkom. hoewel volgehoue studie 'n verband met die voorkoms, van hakkel in verskillende kulture aan te beveel is, sal verklarings vir verskillende verskynsels alleenlik gevind kan word in 'n biolinguistiese benadering tot die aard van die hakkelgedrag onder hierdie verskillende omstandighede. 'n biolinguistiese benadering word aanbeveel, omdat sulke studies 'n verband met die mens en sy gedrag, die lewende organisme bestudeer soos hy gemodifiseer word deur 'n bepaalde omgewing—in hierdie geval grootliks 'n linguistiese omgewing. doel van die studie soos in die geval van vorige navorsing, is dit die doel van hierdie studie om die voorkoms van hakkel te ondersoek, in omgewings waar die taal die belangrikste kulturele onderskeid aantoon. omdat 'n biolinguistiese benadering gevolg word, word egter gepoog om 'n stappie verder te gaan as om net die voorkoms van hakkel te bepaal. dit is nodig om verklarings te gee om aan te toon waarom hierdie verskynsels openbaar word. sulke verklarings kan gevind word in die verband tussen die voorkoms van hakkel en sekere linguistiesfonetiese verskynsels. vorige studies, waarin engelssprekende hakkelaars tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. : des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 68 i. s. uys gebruik is, het reeds bewys dat hakkelaars meer op konsonante, as op vokale, hakkel. in die praktyk is egter gevind dat: 1. in afrikaans die aanvangsvokaal (veral in die geval van 'n beklemtoonde lettergreep) met 'n glottale afsluiting net voor vibrasie, d.w.s. 'n harde aanset, begin word. 2. in engels 'n sagte aanset, d.w.s. 'n geleidelike adduksie van die stemlippe tot by die punt van vibrasie, asook 'n geleidelike aanvang van vibrasie, by aanvangsvokale gebruik word. 3. afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars wel geneig is om dikwels op vokale te hakkel, veral waar hulle in die inisiele posisie van 'n woord of morfeem voorkom. die hipotese, onderliggend aan hierdie eksperimentele studie, is dus dat die harde aanvang of aanset van vokale (d.w.s. glottale afsluiting), by afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars tot 'n hoer frekwensie van hakkel lei, as wat die geval is by engelssprekende hakkelaars, wat die sagte aanvang of aanset gebruik. die benadering tot 'n studie van hierdie aard is fisiologies en linguisties-foneties (dus biolinguisties), omdat die glottale afsluiting en die betekenis en belang daarvan, sowel as sekere kenmerke van foneem-realisering in verskillende tale aandag geniet. 'n studie van die biolinguistiek beklemtoon die harmoniese werking van alle liggaamstelsels in spraaken taaluiting. lenneberg beklemtoon die feit dat taal, soos enige ander gedragspatroon, as 'n manifestasie van ingewikkelde fisiologiese prosesse beskou kan word. die spraakprosesse is dus gemodifiseerde of gefragmenteerde biologiese funksies. tydens spraakproduksie word die spiere teen so 'n vinnige tempo geaktiveer (of gedeaktiveer) dat dit alleenlik deur outomatismes, wat uit ingewikkelde tydspatrone bestaan, uitgevoer kan word. temporale patroonvorming (in teenstelling met temporale wanorde) is dus klaarblyklik op 'n onderliggende fisiologiese ritme gegrond. hierdie feite is veral interessant wanneer dit in die lig van die eienskappe van afrikaans en engels, as spreektale, beskou word. engels is 'n aaneenvloeiende taal, waar sinsritme meer deur intensiteitsmodulasie aangedui word en waar koartikulasie 'n sterk invloed uitoefen. afrikaans daarenteen is meer segmenteel van aard, in die sin dat pouses (selfs tussen lettergrepe) met 'n harde aanset van die daaropvolgende vokaal, algemeen voorkom in die spraakpatroon. hierdie afbakeningsfunksie van die glottale afsluiting bei'nvloed dan ook die spraakritme tot 'n groot mate. y terwyl hakkel as 'n fragmentasie van die spraakproses, of 'n spraakritmeversteuring, beskou word, kan die hipotese gestel word dat hierdie natuurlike neiging tot fragmentasie van spraakritme by sekere tale ook 'n merkbare invloed op die voorkoms van hakkel sal uitoefen. voorafgaande studie hoewel verskeie taalkundiges al op die verskille in vokaalaanset by afrikaans en engels gedui het, is hierdie verskynsels nog nooit weteni journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ondersoek na sekere biolinguistiese verskynsels by hakkel 69 s k a p l i k bepaal nie. 'n voorafgaande studie, om die metode van aanset by verskillende normaalsprekende taalgroepe te bepaal, is dus genoodsaak. vir hierdie deel van die studie is gebruik gemaak van die spraakmonsters wat verkry is van drie groepe proefpersone nl. britse engels, verkry van volwasse persone wat minder as 'n jaar in suidafrika' woonagtig is, suid-afrikaanse engels, verkry van engelssprekende suid-afrikaners en afrikaans, verkry van afrikaanssprekende suid-afrikaners. die spektrograflese analise wat gemaak is van vokaalaanset, is uitsluitlik gebaseer op vokale aan die begin van 'n beklemtoonde lettergreep, hetsy aan die begin van 'n woord, of aan die begin van 'n tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep. klem is konstant gehou aangesien dit 'n faktor is, wat vokaalaanset kan be'invloed. die spektrograflese analise het 'n baie duidelike graadverskil openbaar, sodat die vokaalaanset geklassiflseer kon word onder hard, medium en sag. die resultate dui op 'n sterk verskuiwing van sagte na medium en harde aanset by die drie groepe, in die volgorde: britse engels, suid-afrikaanse engels en afrikaans. die volgende groepneigings kom duidelik na vore. britse engels: 'n baie sterk neiging tot die sagte aanset (75%) word gemerk, veral wanneer dit vergelyk word met die medium (2%) en harde aanset (4%). suid-afrikaanse engels: die gebruik van die sagte aanset (67%) kom ook hier die sterkste na vore, hoewel die medium (30%) en harde aanset (3%) ook gebruik word. afrikaans: die voorkoms van medium (54%) en harde aanset (47%) is byna eweredig versprei by hierdie groep, terwyl die sagte aanset nie eenkeer voorgekom het nie. hoewel hierdie bevindings ooreenstem met vorige gegewens en verwagting, kan daar tog op twee interessante verskynsels gedui word, nl. die wedersydse invloed wat by afrikaans en suid-afrikaanse engels aangetref word en die feit dat die invloed van die inisiele klank op aanset sterker is as die invloed van klem. eksperimentele ontwerp en prosedure proefpersone: volwasse hakkelaars, bo die ouderdom van 16 jaar, is as proefpersone gekies. keuring, op grond van die erns van die hakkel, het geskied op grond van die volgende kriteria: (a) dat die persone hulleself as hakkelaars beskou; (b) dat die eksperimenteerder simptome van onvlotheid, met gepaardgaande gedragsmanifestasies van sekondere hakkel waargeneem het. op hierdie wyse is die resultate verkry van: (i) 12 engelssprekende, suid-afrikaanse manlike hakkelaars. (ii) 11 afrikaanssprekende suid-afrikaners, 9 mans en 2 dames. toetsmateriaal dieselfde toetsmateriaal is gebruik wat in die voorafgaande studie met die normale sprekers gebruik is, ,nl. 'n woordelys bestaande uit 140 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 70 i. s. uys geselekteerde woorde. alledaagse engelse woorde is vir die engelssprekende groep aangebied en bekende afrikaanse woorde vir die afrikaanssprekendes. daar is vir verskeie redes besluit om liewer van woordelyste as van spontane spraak of die lees van sinne, gebruik te maak. (a) daar bestaan nog geen genoegsame bewyse in verband met die voorkoms van wat beskou kan word as 'n aanvangsklank in die middel van 'n sin me. die moontlikheid van ko-artikulasie moet dus geelimineer word. · (b) verskeie veranderlikes, wat die eksperimentele resultate kan beinvloed, moet konstant gehou word, bv. verwagting en posisie van die woord in die sin. (c) die aanbieding van woordelyste kan ook beheer uitoefen oor faktore soos woordlengte, grammatikale funksie, betekenisvolheid en verskeidenheid van aanvangsklanke. (d) terwyl twee taalgroepe betrek is kan die aanbieding van woordelyste sorg dra dat die spraakmonsters op fonetiese grondslag ooreenalgemene prosedure elke proefpersoon is indiwidueel getoets. die woorde is een vir een aangebied en geen tydsbeperking is gestel nie. die beoordeling van die voorkoms van hakkelspasmas is gedoen deur twee opgeleide spraakterapeute, tydens die opname, asook volgens die bandopname. hierdie prosedure is konstant gehou vir alle proefpersone. resultate en bespreking die resultate is deur middel van twee statistiese prosedures verwerk vergelyking binne die groepe is verkry met die wilcoxon simmetrietoets, terwyl vergelyking tussen groepe vasgestel is met die normaalbenadenng vir die verskil tussen twee verhoudings. hierdie toetse het veral geskik geblyk te wees, omdat die studie van 'n relatief klein getal proefpersone gebruik gemaak het. om tot 'n slotsom te kom i.v.m. die hipotese, is die volgende verhoudings statistics bepaal: 1. die verhouding in die voorkoms van hakkel tussen aanvangsvokale en aanvangskonsonante by elke persoon. / 2. die verhouding tussen die twee groepe met betrekking tot die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangsvokale en aanvangskonsonante. 3. die verhouding in die voorkoms van hakkel tussen vokale en konsonante aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreed bv elke persoon. j 4. die verhouding tussen die twee groepe met betrekking tot die voorkoms van hakkel op vokale en konsonante aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no 1: december 197 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ' η ondersoek na sekere biolinguistiese verskynsels by hakkel 71 dit dien vermeld te word dat twee van die afrikaanse hakkelaars vroulik is. vorige navorsing het aangetoon dat laringeale spasmas dikwels by vrouens voorkom, soos ook hier opgemerk kan word uit die resultate van een vroulike proefpersoon. die resultate vir beide vroulike proefpersone is egter behou in die ontleding, eerstens omdat bevind is dat die uitsprake nie verander het wanneer die persone weggelaat word nie, en tweedens om die aantal proefpersone in die groepe naastenby gelyk te hou. 1. die verhouding in die voorkoms van hakkel tussen aanvangsvokale en aanvangskonsonante by elke persoon. t a b e l i : d i e v o o r k o m s v a n h a k k e l o p a a n v a n g s v o k a l e en a a n v a n g s k o n s o n a n t e by e n g e l s s p r e k e n d e h a k k e l a a r s proefpersone aanvangsvokaal aanvangskonsonant totaal l . m 35 24 59 2.m 4 19 23 3.μ 13 14 27 4.μ 18 4 22 5.μ 11 36 47 6.m 18 40 58 7.μ 33 55 88 8.μ 30 39 69 9.μ 3 17 20 10.μ 25 42 67 11.μ 2 10 12 12.μ 11 7 18 203 307 510 uit hierdie tabel kan afgelei word dat die nulhipotese van geen verskil tussen die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangsvokale en aanvangskonsonante op die 5%-peil van betekenis verwerp word. hieruit blyk dit dus dat die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangsvokale betekenisvol minder is as die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangskonsonante. t a b e l i i : d i e v o o r k o m s v a n h a k k e l o p a a n v a n g s v o k a l e en a a n v a n g s k o n s o n a n t e by a f r i k a a n s s p r e k e n d e h a k k e l a a r s proefpersone aanvangsvokale aanvangskonsonante totaal l . m 10 6 16 2.m 2 13 15 3.μ 20 39 59 4.m 5 16 21 5.μ 4 9 13 6.m 53 25 78 7.μ 17 13 30 8.μ 34 2 36 9.μ 4 2 6 10.v 60 6 66 11.v 32 9 41 241 140 •381 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17,nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 72 i. s. uys in hierdie geval word die nulhipotese nie op die 5%-peil van betekenis verwerp nie. die afleiding is dus dat daar geen betekenisvolle verskil bestaan tussen die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangsvokale en aanvangskonsonante nie. 2. die verhouding tussen die twee groepe met betrekking tot die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangsvokale en aanvangskonsonante. in die volgende tabel word 'n uiteensetting gegee van die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangsvokale, om sodoende groepneigings met mekaar te vergelyk. t a b e l i i i : d i e v o o r k o m s v a n h a k k e l o p a a n v a n g s v o k a l e b y b e i d e g r o e p e proefpersone aanvangsvokale engels 203 afrikaans 241 die persentasie hakkel op aanvangsvokale by engelssprekende hakkelaars is dus 24.2%, terwyl dit by afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars 31.3% is. bespreking die verkree waardes dui aan dat daar op die 5%-peil van betekenis wel 'n . verskil bestaan tussen die twee groepe. die afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars het dus betekenisvol meer op aanvangsvokale gehakkel as die engelssprekende hakkelaars. wanneer hierdie resultate vergelyk word met 'n soortgelyke studie wat deur hahn in amerika uitgevoer is, word gevind dat dieselfde neiging, wat in die voorafgaande studie met normale sprekers gevind is, homself hier herhaal. die amerikaanse hakkelaars (wat van 'n sagter aanset gebruik maak as die engelssprekende suid-afrikaanse t a b e l i v : d i e v o o r k o m s v a n h a k k e l o p a a n v a n g s k o n s o n a n t e b y b e i d e g r o e p £ proefpersone aanvangskonsonante engels 307 afrikaans 140 hakkelaars) het slegs op 2.9% van die aanvangsvokale gehakkel. weereens neem die engelssprekende suid-afrikaners die middelgroep in, terwyl die afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars die meeste op aanvangsvokale gehakkel het. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 197ό r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) η ondersoek na sekere biolinguistiese verskynsels by hakkel 73 die persentasie hakkel op aanvangskonsonante by die engelssprekende hakkelaars is dus 36.5% terwyl dit by die afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars 18.2% is. bespreking soos in die geval van aanvangsvokale, is ook hier 'n betekenisvolle verskil gevind tussen die twee groepe. op die 5%-peil van betekenis, het die afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars minder op aanvangskonsonante gehakkel as die engelssprekende hakkelaars. op grond van hierdie betekenisvolle bevindings kan dus afgelei word dat die twee taalgroepe van mekaar verskil met betrekking tot die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangsvokale en aanvangskonsonante. 'n ander motivering vir hierdie stelling word gevind in die vergelyking binne die groepe—dat die engelssprekendes betekenisvol minder op aanvangsvokale gehakkel het as op aanvangskonsonante, terwyl die afrikaanssprekendes geen betekenisvolle verskil toon nie. 3. die verhouding in die voorkoms van hakkel tussen konsonante en vokale aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep, by elke persoon. sekere verskynsels in verband met foneemrealisering en hakkel dien as motivering vir 'n analise van die voorkoms van hakkel op die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep. indien ladefoged se siening korrek is, behoort die begin van die tweede lettergreep (veral omdat dit beklemtoon is) weereens 'n aanvangsklank te he, wat vergelykbaar is met die aanvangsklank by die eerste lettergreep. die feit dat klem tot 'n verhoogde voorkoms van hakkel lei kan ook hier in ag geneem word. indien hierdie verskynsels 'n sterk invloed sou uitoefen op die voorkoms van hakkel, kan verwag word dat dieselfde patroon, as wat gevind is in die geval van aanvangsklanke, homself hier sal herhaal. t a b e l v : d i e v o o r k o m s v a n h a k k e l o p v o k a l e en k o n s o n a n t e a a n d i e b e g i n v a n d i e t w e e d e , b e k l e m t o o n d e l e t t e r g r e e p b y e n g e l s s p r e k e n d e h a k k e l a a r s aanvangsvokaal aanvangskonsonant proefpersone van 2e lettergreep van 2e lettergreep totaal l . m 17 5 22. 2. μ 3 0 3 3.μ 2 0 2 4.μ 0 0 0 5.μ 4 4 8 6.μ 4 10 14 7.μ 12 3 15 8.μ 0 1 1 9.μ 1 2 3 10.μ 12 16 28 11.μ 5 2 7 12.μ 1 1 2 61 44 105 tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des.1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 74 . s. uys die nulhipotese kan hier nie op die 5%-peil van betekenis verwerp word nie, met ander woorde, geen verskil word gevind tussen die voorkoms van hakkel op vokale en konsonante aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep by hierdie groep nie. t a b e l v i : d i e v o o r k o m s v a n h a k k e l o p v o k a l e en k o n s o n a n t e a a n d i e b e g i n v a n d i e t w e e d e , b e k l e m t o o n d e l e t t e r g r e e p by a f r i k a a n s s p r e k e n d e h a k k e l a a r s proefpersone aanvangsvokaa! van 2e lettergreep aanvangskonsonant van 2e lettergreep totaal l . m 3 4 7 2.m 1 2 3 3.μ 3 15 18 4.m 0 2 ' 2 5.μ 4 1 5 6. μ 3 4 7 7.μ 1 3 4 8.μ. 12 4 16 9.μ 1 0 1 1 10.v 15 0 1 16 11.v 17 2 19 60 38 98 net soos in die geval van tabel ii is weereens twee berekenings gedoen (met en sonder die vrouens) en beide berekenings dui daarop dat die nulhipotese nie op die 5%-peil van betekenis verwerp kan word nie. daar bestaan dus geen betekenisvolle verskil tussen die voorkoms van hakkel op konsonante en vokale aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep nie. t a b e l v i i : d i e v o o r k o m s v a n h a k k e l o p v o k a l e a a n d i e b e g i n v a n die t w e e d e , b e k l e m t o o n d e l e t t e r g r e e p b y b e i d e g r o e p e aanvang svokaal proefpersone van 2e lettergreep engels 61 afrikaans 60 in vergelyking met die resultate van tabel ii kan hier gesien word dat dieselfde patroon herhaal word, nl., dat daar in beide gevalle geen betekenisvolle verskille bestaan nie. ./' 4. die verhouding tussen die twee groepe met betrekking tot die voorkoms van hakkel op vokale en konsonante aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep. terwyl die voorafgaande bespreking aangetoon het dat die patroon, wat gevind is in die geval van aanvangsklanke homself alleenlik in die geval van die afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars herhaal, is die twee groepe weer met mekaar vergelyk in 'n pogihg om die groepneigings vas te stel. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. : december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 'η ondersoek na sekere biolinguistiese verskynsels by hakkel 75 tabel vi is 'n aanduiding van die voorkoms van hakkel op vokale aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep en stem ooreen met die analise van aanvangsvokale in tabel iii. die persentasie hakkel op vokale aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep is dus: engels: 25.4% en afrikaans: 27.3%. bespreking volgens analise met die normaalbenadering word gevind dat daar op die 5%-peil van betekenis geen verskil tussen die twee groepe bestaan nie. wat betref die aanvangsvokale is daar wel 'n verskil tussen die twee groepe gevind en wel dat die afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars betekenisvol meer op aanvangsvokale gehakkel het. omdat die spraakmonsters in bogenoemde twee gevalle nie gelykwaardig is nie, is dit nie moontlik om 'n definitiewe vergelyking te tref nie. daar kan egter sekere verskynsels aan die hand gedoen word wat aanleiding kan gee tot hierdie verskille. die sterk neiging onder engelssprekendes om te streef na 'n vloeiende spraakmelodie, wat veral deur vokale oorgedra word, kan aanleiding gee tot die verskynsel van ko-artikulasie, met ander woorde, dat dit moeilik is om te bepaal wanneer daar binne die woord nog 'n aanvangsklank voorkom. die invloed van die inisiele klank blyk dus nog sterker te wees as die invloed van klem op die voorkoms van hakkel. die herhaling van dieselfde patroon in beide gevalle van die afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars is verklaarbaar op grond van die feit dat afrikaans, soos noord-duits 'n afgebroke „springerige" kwaliteit openbaar. kort pouses kom dikwels tussen lettergrepe voor (in teenstelling met engels) en dit kan dui op 'n nuwe aanvang voor 'n tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep. die invloed van die inisiele klank op die voorkoms van hakkel, kan dus in beide gevalle geld. 'n vergelyking tussen die twee groepe met betrekking tot die voorkoms van hakkel op konsonante aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep word in tabel viii gestel. dit is weereens vergelykbaar met die voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangskonsonante soos aangedui in tabel iv. t a b e l v i i i : d i e v o o r k o m s v a n h a k k e l o p k o n s o n a n t e a a n d i e b e g i n v a n d i e t w e e d e , b e k l e m t o o n d e l e t t e r g r e e p by b e i d e g r o e p e aanvangskonsonant proefpersone ' van 2e lettergreep engels 44 afrikaans 38 die persentasie hakkel op konsonante aan die begin van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep is dus: engels: 18.3% en afrikaans 17.3%. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 76 i. s. uys weereens is daar op die 5%-peil van betekenis geen verskil tussen die twee groepe gevind nie. die engelssprekende hakkelaars het egter betekenisvol meer gehakkel op aanvangskonsonante as die afrikaanssprekende groep. dieselfde verklaring wat voorgehou is in die geval van die voorkoms van hakkel op vokale kan moontlik hier van toepassing wees. uit bogenoemde bespreking kan die afleiding dus gemaak word dat, in die geval van die engelssprekende hakkelaars, die aanvangsklank van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep nie nog 'n aanvangsklank in die ware sin van die woord is nie·. die vloeiendheid van die taal, koartikulasie en sinsritme oefen klaarblyklik 'n sterk invloed uit. in afrikaans oefen die neiging tot fragmentasie 'n sterk invloed uit en het die harde aanset 'n sterk afbakeningsfunksie. dit is dus verstaanbaar dat dieselfde patroon hier gevolg word by die aanvang van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep. die hipotese wat gestel is, dat hakkel meer by afrikaanssprekendes sal voorkom op aanvangsvokale, is dus bewys. op grond van die ondersoeke wat uitgevoer is met die 3 groepe normale sprekers, blyk dit ook asof die harde aanset wat by afrikaans voorkom, die oorsaak van hierdie verskynsel is. gevolgtrekkings die primere doel van hierdie studie was om.die invloed van sekere biolinguistiese verskynsels op hakkel te ondersoek. die navorsing is dus ook veral gerig op laringeale funksie onder twee verskillende omstandighede: 1. laringeale funksie in foneem-realisering by twee tale: afrikaans en engels. 2. laringeale funksie as gevolg van foneem-realisering, by hakkel. die resultate bevestig grotendeels die hipotese: 'n sterk verskuiwing van sagte na medium en harde aanset van vokale is waargeneem by die drie groepe normale sprekers, in die volgorde: britse engels, suidafrikaanse engels en afrikaans. dit beaam weereens die stelling dat engels, as spreektaal meer aaneenvloeiend is, waar spraakritme eerder deur intensiteitsmodulasie as deur onderbreking gehandhaaf word. afrikaans daarenteen, toon hoofsaaklik die harde vokaalaanset, wat veral voorkom op aanvangsklanke. aangesien hierdie verskynsel homself herhaal het in die geval van die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep, word hier bevestiging gevind vir die bewering dat afrikaans, as spreektaal, meer segmenteel van aard is, deurdat die afbakeningsfunksie van die glottale afsluiting deur 'n kort pouse voorafgegaan is. die resultate het verder aan die lig gebring dat suid-afrikaanse engels eienskappe van beide britse engels en afrikaans toon. die resultate van die hakkelaars kan dus nou in die lig van die voorkoms van hierdie linguistiese verskynsels verklaar word: afrikaansjournal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. : december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) ' η ondersoek na sekere biolinguistiese verskynsels by hakkel 77 sprekende hakkelaars het betekenisvol meer op aanvangsvokale gehakkel as engelssprekende hakkelaars. die gevolgtrekking kan dus gemaak word dat die glottale afsluiting, wat 'n algemene verskynsel in die afrikaanse spreektaal is, lei tot 'n laringeale hakkelspasma. die relatief sagter aanvang wat deur suid-afrikaanse engelse gebruik word, het betekenisvol minder hakkelspasmas ontlok, terwyl amerikaanse studies bewys dat hakkel selde op aanvangsvokale voorkom. aangesien hierdie selfde patroon homself uitsluitlik in die geval van die afrikaanssprekende hakkelaars op die tweede, beklemtoonde lettergreep herhaal het, bevestig dit die vermoede dat aanvangsvokale in afrikaans ook in die middel van 'n woord kan voorkom. uit hierdie studie blyk dit dus dat 'n biolinguistiese benadering tot 'n verklaring van die voorkoms van hakkel positiewe resultate oplewer. daar is egter nog sekere vrae wat deur verdere navorsing beantwoord moet word: 1. wat is 'n aanvangsklank en wanneer kom dit voor by normale sprekers uit verskillende taalgroepe? 2. sou die resultate dieselfde gewees het indien sinne gelees is of spraakmonsters van spontane spraak verkry is? 3. wat is die frekwensie van laringeale spasmas tydens die voorkoms van hakkel? uit hierdie vrae blyk dit dat daar nog 'n hele nuwe veld braak le. dit kan wees dat daar in die beantwoording van hierdie vrae 'n oplossing vir die raaisel van hakkel gevind kan word. opsomming verskeie navorsingsprojekte het al gepoog om die voorkoms van hakkel in verskillende kulture vas te .stel, sonder om 'n verklaring van die voorkoms en aard van hakkelgedrag deur middel van 'n biolinguistiese benadering aan te bied. linguisties-fonetiese faktore, soos vokale aanvangsmetodes, dui op 'n verskil tussen afrikaans en engels. die harde aanset van aanvangsvokale in afrikaans blyk 'n oorsaak te wees van 'n verhoogde voorkoms van hakkel op aanvangsvokale. 'n biologiese proses (laringeale spanning, glottale afsluiting en verhoogde subglottale lugdruk) aan die een kant en 'n linguistiese verskynsel (die spesifieke eienskappe van foneem-realisering in verskillende tale) aan die ander kant, veroorsaak klaarblyklik die patologiese reaksie (laringeale hakkel). summary various research projects have aimed to explain the occurrence of stuttering amongst various cultural groups, without offering an explanation of the occurrence and nature of the stuttering behaviour in terms of a biolinguistic approach. linguistic phonetic factors, such as vocal initiation, indicate a difference between english and afrikaans. the hard attack on the initial tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse logopediese vereniging, vol. 17, nr. 1: des. 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 7 8 i. s. uys vowels in afrikaans appear to result in an increased frequency of stuttering on these initial vowels. a biological process (laryngeal tension, glottal closure and increased sub-glottal pressure) on the one hand, and a linguistic phenomenon (the specific properties of the phonemic system in different languages) on the other hand, appear to be the cause of the pathological reaction (laryngeal stuttering). bibliografie 1. brown, s. f. -a further study of stuttering in relation to various speech sounds. q.j.s., vol. 24, 1938, p. 390-897. 2. brown, s f . : stuttering with relation to word accent and word position. journal of abnormal and social psychology. vol. 33, 1938, p. 112-130. 3. brown, s. f . : the loci of stuttering in the speech sequence. j.s.d. vol. 10, 1945, p. 181-192. 4. h a h n , e. f . : a study of the relationship between stuttering occurrence and phonetic factors in oral reading. j.s.d. vol. 7, 1942, p. 143-151 5. judson l. s. and weaver, a. t . : voice science. appleton-century-crofts inc., new york, 1965. · 6. ladefoged, p . : three areas of experimental phonetics. oxford university press, london, 1967. 7. le roux, τ. h. and pienaar, p. de v . : afrikaanse fonetiek. juta & kie bpk., kaapstad en johannesburg, 1927. 8. lenneberg, ε. h . : biological foundations of language. john wiley & sons new york, london, sydney, 1967. 9. luchsinger, r. and arnold, g. e . : voice-speech-language. clinical communicology: its physiology and pathology. wadsworth publ. co. inc., belmont, california, 1965. •10. mcdonald ε. t . : articulation: testing and treatment : a sensory-motor approach. stanwix house inc., pittsburgh, 1968 11. mcadcr c l and muyskens, j. h . : a handbook of biohnguistics, part 1. herbert c. weller, toledo speech clinic, inc., ohio 1962. journal of the south african logopedic society, vol. 17, no. 1: december 1970 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 56 sajcd • vol 58 • october 2011 cpd october 2011 1. true (a) or false (b): the music perception test (mpt) was designed to evaluate rhythm, timbre, pitch and melody. 2. true (a) or false (b): audiologists can use the mpt as a counselling tool. 3. true (a) or false (b): all participants performed poorly on the rhythm section of the mpt. 4. true (a) or false (b): a variety of tests evaluating music perception in hearing aid users are available. 5. true (a) or false (b): approximately 15% of individuals with mild traumatic brain injury (mtbi) complain of debilitating symptoms up to 1 year after injury. 6. true (a) or false (b): mtbi will always have a mild impact on an individual’s communication abilities. 7. true (a) or false (b): discourse analysis is a quantitative approach to examine individuals’ perceptions. 8. true (a) or false (b): one of the participants in the mtbi study, p1, viewed loss of temper as a form of communication. 9. true (a) or false (b): irritability and associated loss of temper are always permanent consequences of mtbi. 10. true (a) or false (b): executive function is associated with the frontal lobes of the brain. 11. true (a) or false (b): individuals with mtbi do not experience word retrieval difficulties. 12. true (a) or false (b): the terms dizziness and vertigo both refer to exactly the same symptom. 13. true (a) or false (b): vertigo is a perception of motion when there is an external source for that sensation. 14. true (a) or false (b): the vertigo symptom scale evaluates two areas, namely the experience of dizziness and psychological symptoms. 15. true (a) or false (b): the afrikaans vertigo symptom scale presented with good sensitivity and poor specificity in relation to vertigo. 16. services for people with communication disorders: a) are often well understood b) are well resourced c) often have low priority in health care systems d) are well established worldwide. 17. indigenous knowledge should be used: a) with caution b) to drive the process of development of contextually relevant resources c) in conjunction with external and scientific knowledge to produce locally relevant resources d) b & c. 18. adaptation of a test developed elsewhere: a) is the best way to develop local assessment resources b) is not necessary when used in another context c) could assist in developing local knowledge and resources d) none of the above. 19. studies in the local context are necessary to: a) determine the prevalence of communication disorders in south africa b) obtain legislative support for service delivery c) describe the status of services currently available d) all of the above. 20. speech-language therapists in public hospitals: a) fulfil different roles in neonatal nurseries, as determined by their context b) have sufficient time to participate in ward rounds c) are all involved in discharge planning d) all of the above. cpd questionnaires must be completed online via www.cpdjournals.co.za. after submission you can check the answers and print your certificate. the south african journal of communication disorders sajcd sajcd 120 the occurrence of high-risk factors for hearing loss in very-low-birth-weight neonates: a retrospective exploratory study of targeted hearing screening a kanji, k khoza-shangase   department of speech pathology and audiology, school of human and community development, university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg amisha kanji katijah khoza-shangase corresponding author: a kanji (amisha.kanji@wits.ac.za) the current study aimed at determining the type and frequency of high-risk factors for hearing loss in a group of very-low-birth-weight (vlbw) neonates in a tertiary hospital in south africa with the objective of collating evidence that could be used in arguing for or against revisiting targeted hearing screening in developing countries. furthermore, the study aimed at investigating the relationship between the identified high-risk factors and hearing screening results. in a retrospective data review design, data were collated from files from the vlbw project; this included hearing screening records, as well as records from participant medical and audiology files. records of 86 neonates with birth weights ranging between 680 g and 1 500 g were reviewed. findings indicated that neonatal jaundice, exposure to human immunodeficiency virus (hiv), mechanical or assisted ventilation, and neonatal intensive care unit stay greater than 48 hours were the most frequently occurring high-risk factors for hearing loss in the current sample. these factors are consistent with those listed in the high-risk register of the health professions council of south africa for the south african context. findings confirm the complexity of risk factors, and the influence that a variety of factors such as poor follow-up or return rate might have on the implementation of early hearing detection and intervention. the importance of establishing context-specific risk factors for effective implementation of targeted screening protocols where universal newborn hearing screening is not yet a reality was highlighted by the current study. keywords: hearing loss, risk factors, very low birth weight, neonates, targeted newborn screening, developing country s afr j cd 2012;59(1):3-7. doi:10.7196/sajcd.120 there has been a considerable increase in the survival rate of very-low-birth-weight (vlbw, <1 500 g) neonates over the last few decades as a result of improvements in medical care (ruegger, hegglin, adams & bucher, 2012). this increase is particularly recorded in developed countries where improved outcome following medical intervention in the neonatal intensive care unit (nicu) has been well documented (darlow, cust & donoghue, 2003). while these improvements and advances are readily adopted by developing countries, implementation is often accompanied by poorly resourced health services (ballot, potterton, chirwa, hilburn & cooper, 2012).within the south african context, an overall survival rate of 72% among vlbw neonates admitted to a public sector hospital in gauteng has been reported (velaphi et al., 2005). this increase in survival rate raises concern regarding the associated, increased rates of neurodevelopmental disability among these vlbw neonates (claas et al., 2011). rates of disability among surviving vlbw neonates may be higher in developing countries such as south africa (ballot, chirwa & cooper, 2010) as the setting and resources in these contexts differ markedly to those in developed countries. factors such as increased length of hospital stay (mokhachane, saloojee & cooper, 2006), and increased risk of the surviving neonate being subjected to a variety of complications while in hospital (wood et al., 2000) are known to result in a range of problems such as cerebral palsy, cognitive impairment, blindness and hearing impairment (hack, 2007). although vlbw in isolation has not been documented to have a severe impact on hearing, it is frequently associated with multiple risk factors that can affect hearing in a collective manner (cristobal & oghalai, 2008). the probability of sensorineural hearing loss has been found to increase as the number of coexisting risk factors increases, with the probability being nearly double for those with five or more risk factors for hearing loss (bielecki, horbulewicz & wolan, 2011). these risk factors may differ across communities and contexts (olusanya, luxon & wirz, 2004) and there is paucity of data on the high-risk factors for hearing loss among vlbw neonates within the south african context. establishing the rate of occurrence of these risk factors in this population can contribute towards efforts aimed at early identification of hearing loss. benefits of early identification of hearing loss have been well documented, and include increased access to more prompt and appropriate intervention (hpcsa, 2007; jcih, 2007). evidence of positive benefits from early hearing detection and intervention (ehdi) led to the establishment of a high-risk register by the joint committee on infant hearing (jcih) (kountakis, skoulas, phillips & chang, 2002). although the use of the register as a sole screening method has limitations, such as missing 25 50% of neonates with hearing loss (kountakis et al., 2002), it is believed to be useful as a referral protocol and necessary for the identification of infants who may require monitoring and follow-up screening (johnson, 2002). it can be argued that this necessity holds particularly true in contexts where universal newborn hearing screening is not yet feasible, such as in most developing countries. the risk factors for permanent congenital and early-onset hearing loss documented by the jcih are usually adopted in such screening programmes, but these may be expanded to include other risk factors appropriate to the context, especially in developing countries (olusanya et al., 2004). modifying risk factors to make them context-relevant is crucial as the literature has shown that adoption of an evidence-based model of care allows for best practice. for example, kountakis et al. (2002), based on findings from their study conducted at hermann hospital in houston, texas, identified 11 variables not included in the jcih (1994) high-risk register which had a statistically significant correlation to hearing loss in their context. the jcih (2007) high-risk register has since been updated to include a few of these risk factors. although the hpcsa (2007) position statement on ehdi has led to the initiation of newborn hearing screening programmes in both the public and private healthcare sectors in the country, these programmes remain mostly unstructured, disorganised and uncommon because they are unauthorised and not mandated by hospital management in these sectors (swanepoel, storbeck & friedland, 2009). the lack of success in implementation of newborn hearing screening in south africa can be attributed to a number of factors. firstly, priorities within the health sector are focused on saving lives rather than addressing quality of life in individuals with non-threatening conditions such as hearing loss (swanepoel, hugo & louw, 2006). secondly, when assessing the number of qualified audiologists in the country in relation to population size, there is an evident shortage of manpower in the public healthcare sector (olusanya et al., 2004) which makes achievement of goals of early detection of hearing loss and early intervention difficult, unless middle-level workers and/or nurses are trained to perform hearing screening. the hpcsa (2007) has recommended a list of high-risk factors to be used for targeted or risk-based screening. these high-risk factors are based on those specified in the year 2000 jcih position statement for ehdi programmes, with two additional risk factors that are considered contextually relevant to the south african context, namely hiv and malaria (hpcsa, 2007). reviewed evidence indicates that risk factors for permanent congenital and early-onset hearing loss may vary across communities. current authors support the view that one should not consider the risk factors listed by the jcih with the same relative importance because of considerable variation of situations and time periods in different countries (korres et al., 2005). we support olusanya’s (2008) argument that a need exists for developing countries to be guided by empirical evidence on the relevant risk factors for each community and population when making the decision to embark on targeted screening, hence the importance of the current study in the vlbw population within a south african context. while the reviewed studies have focused on neonates in the nicu and well-baby nurseries, no studies pertaining to high-risk factors have been conducted in a developing country like south africa where vlbw has been reported to contribute significantly to the total number of neonatal admissions. furthermore, in developing countries where there are limited healthcare resources and high patient numbers, it is often not possible to provide full tertiary support to every vlbw neonate (ballot et al., 2010). this reality highlights the need for further research in the vlbw population cared for in other settings (i.e. high care, low care or kangaroo mother care), where the need for newborn hearing screening has not yet been identified and prioritised, or published as evidence from developing countries. the identification of the type and occurrence of high-risk factors for hearing loss in vlbw neonates is one such important research area which can assist audiologists with the development of appropriate, efficient and sensitive targeted hearing screening protocols, particularly when manpower shortages prevent or restrict screening of all neonates in all neonatal wards. method aims the main aim was to describe which of the hpcsa (2007) high-risk factors for hearing loss were present in a group of vlbw neonates. secondary objectives were to determine the occurrence of these risk factors in the sample, and to establish whether any one or combination of these risk factors was independently or jointly related to distortion product oto-acoustic emissions (dpoae) screening results. research context the study was conducted at a tertiary level hospital in gauteng, south africa. the hospital has a fully operational nicu and established neonatal clinics, as well as an audiology department. participants inclusion criteria inclusion criteria stipulated that participants had to have been part of the vlbw project, weighing 1 500 g or less, with complete hearing screening records for analysis, and must have had the initial hearing screening within the neonatal period. sample the sample comprised 86 participants (35 males and 51 females) with a gestational age range of 26 40 weeks (mean = 31 weeks). the birth weight range was 680 1 500 g (mean = 1 199 g). participants had been a part of the vlbw project which was a longitudinal study aimed at determining the functional and developmental outcomes of vlbw infants 12 15 months of corrected age. these participants had been assessed at follow-up visits by a paediatrician, nurse and allied medical disciplines. design the current study employed a passive, archival, quantitative research design as it involved a retrospective record review with no manipulation of variables, and the researcher used existing documents to analyse variables across time and condition (devlin, 2006). data collection and analysis data were obtained from archived hearing screening records which were part of a vlbw project that was conducted between july 2006 and february 2007. the hpcsa (2007) high-risk register was used to identify potential risk factors for hearing loss for each participant in the current retrospective study. each identified risk factor was recorded next to each participant’s code. data for each participant were collated onto an excel spread sheet for ease of data handling and analysis. ethical considerations ethical clearance (protocol number: m060546) was obtained from the medical research ethics committee and informed consent was obtained from caregivers for the vlbw project. data were only utilised retrospectively for the current study following approval from the medical research ethics committee (protocol number: m090565), as well as permission from the postgraduate committee. for the purposes of the current study, confidentiality and anonymity of the participants was maintained by ensuring that a research coding system was utilised instead of participant names and hospital identity numbers. reliability and validity owing to the current study being a retrospective record review, reliability and consistency of case history data were maintained by ensuring that data were obtained from medical record reviews rather than caregivers’ reports. limited patient recollection of events may result in recall bias (panacek, 2007). similarly, inaccuracies of medical records may also occur (panacek, 2007). the current study ensured accurate case history data by cross-checking the information recorded on the speech, hearing and feeding assessment form to the original nicu admission records. standardisation of ‘pass’/‘refer’ criteria was also maintained throughout analysis of dpoae screening results. validity was enhanced by considering the influence of environmental and patient factors that could affect dpoae screening results. therefore, frequencies below 1 khz were eliminated from statistical analysis because these frequencies are most affected by acoustic ambient noise, and external and internal artefacts. reliability and validity of the test protocol adopted for the vlbw project was deemed appropriate. hearing screening had been performed through the use of dpoae through the biologic audx dpoae screener. participants who did not pass the initial test in one or both ears, as well as those who were discharged before completion of screening, were referred to the audiology department 6 weeks after discharge (which corresponded to their neonatal follow-up), for a follow-up screening. those participants who passed the initial screening were also referred for a follow-up screening. data analysis data were collated and tabulated nominally. this was done in order to identify the dominant trends which emerged in relation to high-risk factors. descriptive statistics were then utilised to illustrate and make sense of findings. data obtained were compared against the high-risk factors for hearing loss as defined by the hpcsa (2007). analysis of a relationship between the most frequently occurring risk factors and dpoae screening results was also performed using two-way contingency tables and the chi-square test. analysis using all five risk factors in combination was not performed because of sample size constraints. ‘pass’/‘refer’ criteria for the analysis of dpoae results were adopted. initial dpoae screening results were descriptively analysed by frequency, as either ‘pass’ or ‘refer’. owing to reported high ambient noise levels in a hospital (olusanya, somefun & swanepoel, 2008; olusanya, wirz & luxon, 2008), which primarily affect the low frequencies, 250 hz and 500 hz, 750 hz and 1 000 hz were not included within the ‘pass’/‘refer’ criteria. reliable data using dpoaes should be expected at 2, 3 and 4 khz (gorga et al., 2000); therefore, in the current study, ‘pass’/‘refer’ criteria were assessed using 2, 3, 4, 6 and 8 khz. an overall ‘pass’ result required a unilateral or bilateral ‘pass’ result at, at least four of the five frequencies. results a summary of the findings from the current study is depicted in table 1. table 1. frequency of occurrence of risk factors among vlbw neonates ( n =86)   frequency percentage risk factors for hearing loss as indicated by the hpcsa (2007)     neonatal jaundice 76 88.37 hiv exposed 15 17.44 nicu stay greater than 48 hours 13 15.11 mechanical/assisted ventilation 13 15.11 exposure to ototoxic medication 9 10.46 associated syndrome (11th chromosome deletion) 1 1.16 syphilis 1 1.16 other risk factors (not specific to hearing loss) present in data of study sample prematurity 85 98.83 hmd 18 20.93 ivh grade ii 9 10.46 hypoxia/birth asphyxia 8 9.30 renal dysfunction 8 9.30 hyperglycaemia 5 5.81 eclampsia 4 4.65 ivh grade i 3 3.49 hypoglycaemia 3 3.49 anaemia 2 2.32 choriamnionitis 2 2.32 ivh grade iii 1 1.16 vlbw = very low birth weight; nicu = neonatal intensive care unit; hmd = hyaline membrane disease; ivh = intraventricular haemorrhage. from the risk factors stipulated by the hpcsa (2007) high-risk register, neonatal jaundice (88.37%) was the most frequently occurring risk factor found in the current study, followed by exposure to hiv (17.44%), nicu stay for more than 48 hours (15.11%) and mechanical or assisted ventilation (15.11%). of participants presenting with neonatal jaundice (n=76), 31 (41%) were male, 25 were recorded as having received phototherapy and only 3 received exchange blood transfusions. with regard to hiv status, the remainder of the sample presented with 45 participants who were not exposed to hiv, 16 whose hiv status was unknown and 10 whose caregivers refused informed consent for hiv testing. the other risk factors for hearing loss listed by the hpcsa (2007) had a frequency of less than 15% as seen in table 1. other risk factors reported in the literature and believed to be clinically significant although not listed on the hpcsa (2007) register were prematurity (98.83%), birth asphyxia or hypoxia (9.30%), hypoglycaemia (3.49%) and hyperglycaemia (5.81%) as indicated in table 1. in the current study, prematurity was found to be the most frequently occurring among the other risk factors which were thought to be clinically significant. some significant risk factors occurred in combination with each other. for example, 3 participants were premature with exposure to hiv, 54 were premature with neonatal jaundice, while 1 had exposure to hiv with neonatal jaundice. further analysis revealed that 8 participants presented with a combination of three risk factors: neonatal jaundice, prematurity and exposure to hiv (7); prematurity, neonatal jaundice and mechanical or assisted ventilation (1). a combination of four risk factors was present in 10 participants. findings from the hearing screening through dpoae revealed that of the total baseline sample of 86 neonates who were expected at follow-up, only 27 returned for a follow-up, outpatient screening. all 86 participants presented with a combination of risk factors but most were found to pass the initial and follow-up screening (figure 1), suggesting a lower referral rate among the vlbw participants included in the current study. of the 27 participants who returned for follow-up screening, 15 passed the initial and follow-up screening. fig. 1. results for initial and follow-up distortion product oto-acoustic emissions (dpoae) screening. chi-square analysis using 5% level of significance (α=0.05) revealed a relationship that was not statistically significant (χ²<5.99) between the most frequently occurring risk factors and dpoae screening results. the most frequently occurring risk factors were included in the analysis independently, as well as in combination with each other. these risk factors included prematurity, neonatal jaundice and nicu stay for greater than 48 hours, exposure to hiv and mechanical/assisted ventilation. results indicate that whether these most frequent risk factors existed in isolation or in combination, the overall screening results were not influenced. records of these participants having undergone diagnostic evaluations were not present in the patient files, and this had been hypothesised to be possibly due to poor follow-up attendance, an important indirect finding which emanated from the current study. discussion in achieving the aim of identifying the type and occurrence of risk factors for hearing loss in a group of vlbw neonates, the current study revealed the presence of five most frequently occurring risk factors. four of these risk factors (neonatal jaundice requiring exchange blood transfusion, mechanical/assisted ventilation, nicu stay greater than 48 hours and exposure to hiv) are listed on the hpcsa (2007) high-risk register. the fifth risk factor thought to be clinically significant in the current study was prematurity. this is in contrast to a study conducted in kuwait, where it was found that of the 105 newborns, birth weight below or equivalent to 1 500 g, ototoxic medications, mechanical ventilation for greater than 5 days and meningitis were the most prevalent risk factors (al-harbi, barakat & al-khandary, 2008). these findings highlight that although both these studies were conducted in developing countries and even with the presence of guidelines for high-risk factors for hearing loss, differences in type, frequency and occurrence of risk factors for hearing loss still exist. hence, continual investigation of the relative importance of specific high-risk factors is necessary for assessment, refinement and modification of clinical protocols to ensure clinical practice that is relevant to the context (korres et al., 2005). the most frequently occurring high-risk factor in the current study was neonatal jaundice, presenting in more than half of the total sample, with a higher frequency in females than males. the higher occurrence in females is contrary to findings which documented an observed higher risk of neonatal jaundice in males (olusanya, akande, emokpae & olowe, 2009). this finding may however be influenced by the fact that the current study sample comprised more females than males, and it therefore should be interpreted with caution. the high percentage of neonates presenting with neonatal jaundice in the current study is also consistent with reports that state that the burden of neonatal jaundice is likely to be substantially higher in africa compared with the developed world (olusanya et al., 2009). although hyperbilirubinaemia requiring exchange blood transfusion is listed as a risk factor for hearing loss, the frequency of neonatal jaundice necessitating phototherapy was greater than exchange blood transfusion. this is consistent with reports from another developing country, nigeria, where the need for phototherapy reportedly exceeded exchange blood transfusion, with those who received phototherapy also being at significant risk for sensorineural hearing loss (olusanya et al., 2009). these findings from the current study may have also been influenced by the unit policies at the hospital during this time period, whereby all neonates, irrespective of birth weight, were provided with standard neonatal care which included blood transfusions or phototherapy as needed (ballot et al., 2010). prematurity (although not listed on the hpcsa (2007) high-risk register) was also a frequently occurring coexisting risk factor with neonatal jaundice in the current study. it is assumed that the earlier the occurrence of neonatal hyperbilirubinaemia, the more likely it is to affect the auditory pathways and it is therefore thought that preterm infants have a higher risk of developing hearing impairment, even with lower bilirubin levels (nickisch, massinger, ertl-wagner & von voss, 2009). this has clinical significance as it highlights the importance of close monitoring of preterm neonates with coexisting risk factors, as well as the clinical importance of using both otoacoustic emissions and automated auditory brainstem response in neonates with neonatal jaundice to ensure that auditory neuropathy is not missed. in the current study of vlbw neonates, exposure to hiv was only present in 17.44% of the neonates. this unexpected finding is contrary to reports which state an association between hiv and an increased risk of low birth weight (rollins, coovadia, bland, patel & newell, 2007). owing to the fact that in the current study, the hiv status could not be established in approximately one-fourth of the sample, the authors acknowledge that this finding is only an approximate and may be influenced by sample size since some caregivers in the current study refused to give consent for hiv testing. although the risk of hearing loss is reported to increase with the number of existing risk factors, the true impact of the combination of risk factors and their cumulative effect on hearing outcome could not be clearly established because of poor follow-up return rate, the small study sample size and a lack of diagnostic data. poor follow-up return rate further precluded the confirmation of hearing impairment in this population, as well as gleaning of information regarding intervention. this highlights the need for audiologists to ensure that efforts are made to improve follow-up return rate (kanji, khoza-shangase & ballot, 2010). the sample size of the current study was small and was further reduced because of incomplete medical information and screening results, which resulted in the final sample being too small for generalisability of the results to larger and broader contexts. the inclusion of high-frequency tympanometry testing and automated auditory brainstem response testing could have added another dimension to the current study, as it would have assisted in distinguishing between conductive, sensory and neural types of hearing impairment. false-positive and false-negative findings could have impacted on the screening results and hence influenced the results related to the correlation between the type of risk factors and dpoae screening results. the analyses performed in the current study are exploratory and should only be used as a basis for further research. although exploratory, these findings do highlight that although vlbw is not considered a risk factor for hearing loss, this population does present with multiple risk factors for hearing loss as listed on the hpcsa (2007) high-risk register. this therefore has clinical implications for the audiologist who needs to ensure that the vlbw population forms part of the priority client load that should be monitored, even though vlbw on its own is not considered as a risk factor for hearing loss, i.e. targeted hearing screening needs to extend beyond the nicu to high care, low care and kangaroo mother care wards where vlbw neonates may be admitted instead. conclusions the list of risk indicators for hearing loss still requires constant modification and more detailed categorisation in terms of severity as risk factors may be influenced by the resources, community and diseases present in different contexts during different time periods. the less frequently occurring risk factors need to be investigated further by audiologists as this may lead to growing evidence regarding the inclusion of additional risk factors on the high-risk register – one that is context-specific and context-relevant, ensuring appropriate, early referrals among relevant medical professionals and audiologists. even though findings from the current study cannot be generalised to the larger population, and even though conclusions regarding the association between high-risk factors and hearing loss cannot be drawn without further analyses, current findings call for further research in this population taking into consideration current limitations. acknowledgements. some aspects of the vlbw project pertaining to follow-up return rate have been published. we would like to thank prof. peter fridjhon for his statistical assistance as well as prof. daynia ballot from the department of paediatrics and child health. references al-harbi, m., barakat, n., & al-khandary, m. (2008). hearing screening in at risk newborn. journal of medical sciences, 8(7), 648-653. ballot, d. e., chirwa, t. f., & cooper, p. a. (2010). determinants of survival in very low birth weight neonates in a public sector hospital in johannesburg. bmc pediatrics, 10, 30. doi:10.1186/1471-2431-10-30 ballot, d. e., potterton, j., chirwa, t., hilburn, n., & cooper, p. a. (2012). developmental outcome of very low birth weight infants in a developing country. bmc pediatrics, 12(11), 1-10. doi:10.1186/1471-2431-12-11 bielecki, i., horbulewicz, a., & wolan, t. (2011). risk factors associated with hearing loss in infants: an analysis of 5282 referred neonates. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 75(7), 925-930. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2011.04.007 claas, m. j., bruinse, h. w., koopman, c., van haastert, i. c., peelen, l. m., & de vries, l. s. (2011). two-year neurodevelopmental outcome of preterm born children ≤ 750 g at birth. archives of disease in childhood fetal and neonatal edition, 96(3), f169-f177. cristobal, r., & oghalai, j. s. (2008). hearing loss in children with very low birth weight: current review of epidemiology and pathophysiology. archives of disease in childhood fetal and neonatal edition, 93, f462-f468. doi:10.1136/adc.2007.124214 darlow, b. a., cust, a. e., & donoghue, d. a. (2003). improved outcomes for very low birthweight infants: evidence from new zealand national population based data. archives of disease in childhood fetal and neonatal edition, 88, f23-f28. doi:10.1136/fn.88.1.f23 devlin, a. s. (2006). research methods: planning, conducting and presenting research. belmont: thomson wadsworth. gorga, m. p., norton, s. j., sininger, y. s., cone-wesson, b., folson, r. c., vohr, b. r., et al. (2000). identification of neonatal hearing impairment: distortion product otoacoustic emissions during the perinatal period. ear and hearing, 21(5), 400-424. doi:10.1097/00003446-200010000-00007 hack, m. (2007). survival and neurodevelopmental outcomes of preterm infants. journal of pediatric gastroenterology and nutrition, 45(suppl 3), s141-s142. doi:10.1097/01.mpg.0000302959.55428.05 hpcsa. (2007). early hearing detection and intervention programmes in south africa, position statement year 2007. retrieved from http://www.hpsca.co.za/hpcsa/default.aspx?id=137 jcih. (1994). joint committee on infant hearing: 1994 position statement. retrieved from http://www.jcih.org/jcih1994.pdf jcih (2000). year 2000 position statement: principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. retrieved from http://www.jcih.org/jcih2000.pdf jcih. (2007). year 2007 position statement: principles and guidelines for early hearing detection and intervention programs. pediatrics, 120(4), 898-921. johnson, c. d. (2002). special populations. in j. kats, r. f. bukard & l. medwetsky (eds.), handbook of clinical audiology (5th ed., pp. 481-494). baltimore: lippincott williams & wilkins. kanji, a., khoza-shangase, k., & ballot, d. (2010). hearing screening follow-up return rate in a very low birth weight project: a retrospective record review. south african journal of child health, 4(4), 95-99. korres, s., nikolopoulos, t. p., komkotou, v., balatsouras, d., kandiloros, d., constantinou, d., et al. (2005). newborn hearing screening: effectiveness, importance of high-risk factors, and characteristics of infants in the neonatal intensive care unit and well-baby nursery. otology neurotology, 26, 1186-1190. doi:10.1097/01.mao.0000184602.94677.41 kountakis, s. e., skoulas, i., phillips, d., & chang, c. y. (2002). risk factors for hearing loss in neonates: a prospective study. american journal of otolaryngology, 23(3), 133-137. doi:10.1053/ajot.2002.123453 mokhachane, m., saloojee, h., & cooper, p. a. (2006). earlier discharge of very low birthweight infants from an under-resourced african hospital: a randomized trial. annals of tropical paediatrics, 26, 43-51. nickisch, a., massinger, c., ertl-wagner, b., & von voss, h. (2009). pedaudiologic findings after severe neonatal hyperbilirubinemia. european archives of otolaryngology, 266, 207-212. doi:10.1007/s00405-008-0737-2 olusanya, b. o. (2008). priorities for early hearing detection and intervention in sub saharan africa. international journal of audiology, 47(suppl 1), s3-s13. doi:10.1080/14992020802287143 olusanya, b. o., akande, a. a., emokpae, a., & olowe, s. a. (2009). infants with severe neonatal jaundice in lagos, nigeria: incidence, correlates and hearing screening outcomes. tropical medicine and international health, 14(3), 301-310. doi:10.1111/j.1365-3156.2009.02223.x olusanya, b. o., luxon, l. m., & wirz, s. l. (2004). benefits and challenges of newborn hearing screening for developing countries. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 68, 287-305. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2003.10.015 olusanya, b. o., somefun, a. o., & swanepoel, d. (2008). the need for standardization of methods for worldwide infant hearing screening: a systematic review. the laryngoscope, 118, 1830-1836. doi:10.1097/mlg.0b013e31817d755e olusanya, b. o., wirz, s. l., & luxon, l. m. (2008). hospital-based universal newborn hearing screening for early detection of permanent congenital hearing loss in lagos, nigeria. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 72, 991-1001. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2008.03.004 panacek, e. a. (2007). performing chart review studies. air medical journal, 26(5), 206-210. doi:10.1016/j.amj.2007.06.007 rollins, n. c., coovadia, h. m., bland, r. m., patel, d., & newell, m. l. (2007). pregnancy outcomes in hiv-infected and uninfected women in rural and urban south africa. journal of acquired immune deficiency syndrome, 44(3), 321-328. doi:10.1097/qai.0b013e31802ea4b0 ruegger, c., hegglin, m., adams, m., & bucher, h. u. (2012). population based trends in mortality, morbidity and treatment for very preterm and very low birth weight infants over 12 years. bmc pediatrics, 12(12), 1-12. doi:10.1186/1471-2431-12-17 swanepoel, d., hugo, r., & louw, b. (2006). infant hearing screening at immunization clinics in south africa. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 70(6), 1241-1249. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2006.01.002 swanepoel, d., storbeck, c., & friedland, p. (2009). early hearing detection and intervention in south africa. international journal of pediatric otorhinolaryngology, 73(6), 783-786. doi:10.1016/j.ijporl.2009.01.007 velaphi, s. c., mokhachane, m., mphahlele, r. m., beckh-arnold, e., kuwanda, m. l., & cooper, p. a. (2005). survival of very-low-birth-weight infants according to birth weight and gestational age in a public hospital. south african medical journal, 95(7), 504-509. wood, n. s., marlow, n., costeloe, k., chir, b., gibson, a. t., & wilkinson, a. r. (2000). neurologic and developmental disability after extremely preterm birth. new england journal of medicine, 343, 378-384. doi:10.1056/nejm200008103430601 . a new theory on the relationship of stuttering and handedness. (an extract from a paper presented for the degree of b.a. log. at the university of the witwatersrand). patricia r. samuels. many investigations have "been carried out on the relationship of laterality and stuttering, and the majority of these are familiar to all speech therapists. however, recent investigators have forsaken the long established association postulated "by travis and his co-workers, and have introduced a new concept. the relationship reported by these investigators is not one of cause and effect, "but one which pre-supposes a common causal factor for "both left-handedness and stuttering. this theory has "been investigated "by berry, west, neilson, hunter and others and is "based on twinning. berry in an attempt to find a common denominator for twinning and stuttering collected data on 250 duplicate "births. the problem she presented was "does stuttering appear more frequently in twinning families than in families without records of duplicate "births?". the questionnaire covered points in regard to history of handedness in direct and collateral lines and & history of speech defects in the same manner. in respect of the present discussion she found that in regard to sinistrality in these families, representing 1,205 children, approximately 1 in 10 was left-handed. jones reported that in the united states b% of the population was left-handed; there are, therefore, two-and-a-half times more sinistrals in the twinning families than in the general population. as regards speech in twinning families, there were 66 stutterers in these 1250 twinning families. this represents 5*5$ of the total number of children and is therefore much higher than the normal expectancy, for in this case expectancy would "be 1 in 18, instead of the normal expectancy of 1 in every 100 children. in an attempt to find a common denominator berry states that the frequency of left-handedness, of retardation in the onset and development of speech, and in the case of twinning, a higher incidence of stuttering is greater in these families. thus it may "be postulated, on the assumption that lefthandedness is inherited, that stuttering too, is due to heredity. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 33.. twinning, stuttering and left-handedness seem to occur together and to have properties in common. the single siblings in these families tend to have a norm of one (l) in 35 being stutterers. this is very high and, therefore, contra-indicates the assumption that twinning per se produced stuttering or that left-handedness per se produced stuttering. the factor in the germ plasm producing twins may be a gene which establishes an abnormal biochemical relationship "favourable to the retardation of a single axiate production, and unfavourable to normal speech". is it possible that the genie force, favourable to twinning and stuttering, may show itself in some process connected with vitamin metabolism?. the theory just expressed may be reiterated for handedness. left-handedness and twinning may be part of the same basic phenomenon, although they have no direct effect on each other. there are many theories which we may follow in establishing a common basis for left-handedness, twinning and stuttering. for example, if we followed tauterbach's hypothesis, it may well be that left-handed stutterers lost their mates in utero, they were, in origin, twins. another line of reasoning which may be pursued is that of hkwman it might be argued that the interference with fetal circulation, which may occur in twinning, could affect both the centres normally dominant in establishing speach and handedness. "all in all, whatever "theory one accepts, the best answer to the question of sinistrality, stuttering and twinning seems to reside in a genie constitution". neilson, hunter and walker in a study on stuttering in twin types found that the percentage amongst 200 twin pairs was 20%, which is very much higher than that for the general population. west, neilson and berry state that "there are three groups which are innately atypical: the stutterers, the lefthanders and the twinners. they are by no means identical but there is considerable overlapping. in view of the strongly hereditary factors of all three it is difficult to think of one as being the cause of the others; one is inclined rather to suppose that all three rest upon some common, hereditable factor of structure or biochemistry". thus these investigators postulate that these two rarities of socity, stuttering and left-handedness, may be linked by one causal factor, a genie force as yet unknown to science. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3k· ' references. berry, m. west, neilβon & berry neilson, hunter & walker "a common denominator in twinning and stuttering". (j.s.d. march 1938). "heredity of stuttering". (q.j.s. 1939). "stuttering in twin types (j.s.d. december 19*4-5) r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) speech and the laryngectomized by mary a. doehler d i r e c t o r of e s o p h o g e a l s p e e c h , m a s s a c h u s e t t s e y e a n d e a r i n f i r m a r y , b o s t o n , m a s s a c h u s e t t s s i n c e s p e e c h is t h e m o s t c h a r a c t e r i s t i c h u m a n a c t a n d t h e f i r s t m e a n s b y w h i c h s o c i a l r e l a t i o n s h i p s a r e e s t a b l i s h e d a n d k e p t i n t a c t , i t s l o s s s e r i o u s l y t h r e a t e n s t h e l a r y n g e c t o m i z e d p a t i e n t ' s f e e l i n g of s e c u r i t y a n d b a l a n c e , b o t h i n t h e f a m i l y a n d i n t h e c o m m u n i t y . t o c o m p l e t e a s a t i s f a c t o r y r e h a b i l i t a t i o n o r a d j u s t m e n t i n v o l v e s s e v e r a l s t e p s , n o n e of w h i c h s h o u l d b e n e g l e c t e d . i s h a l l d i s c u s s t h e s e f a c t o r s a s i s e e t h e m , in o r d e r of t h e i r i m p o r t a n c e t o t h e p a t i e n t , a s i a t t e m p t t o e v a l u a t e t h e t e a c h i n g p r o g r a m . 1. the mental and emotional preparation of the pupil prior to surgery t h e r e c a n b e n o s e t r u l e s e s t a b l i s h e d in t h i s p r e o p e r a t i v e p r e p a r a t i o n s i n c e e a c h p e r s o n ' s r e a c t i o n t o a n y c r i s i s o r s t r a i n is d e t e r m i n e d o r m e a s u r e d b y o n e ' s o w n p e r s o n a l i t y s t r u c t u r e , a n d o n e ' s o w n w a y i n p r e v i o u s l y d e a l i n g w i t h c r i s e s . m a n y t i m e s t h e s u r g e o n w i l l p r o v i d e t h e n e c e s s a r y p s y c h o l o g i c a l h e l p . a g a i n , a w e l l t r a i n e d s p e e c h t h e r a p i s t c a n b e of g r e a t v a l u e t o t h e p r o s p e c t i v e p a t i e n t . i n a n y c a s e , t h e p r e p a r a t i o n s h o u l d b e b a s e d u p o n a c r i t i c a l e v a l u a t i o n of t h e p e r s o n a l i t y of t h e p a t i e n t t h r o u g h a p e r s o n a l i n t e r v i e w . i t is a d v i s a b l e a t t h i s t i m e t o i n c l u d e a m e m b e r of t h e f a m i l y . d u r i n g t h e i n t e r v i e w t h e p u p i l s h o u l d b e e n c o u r a g e d t o d i s c u s s t h e m a n y p r o b l e m s h e w i l l h a v e t o face. n a t u r a l l y , t h e l o s s of h i s v o i c e a n d t h e r e s u l t i n g s o c i a l a n d e c o n o m i c p r o b l e m s a r e t h e m o s t s e r i o u s . if p o s s i b l e , it is a d v a n t a g e o u s a t t h i s p o i n t t o h a v e t h e p u p i l m e e t s o m e o n e a b o u t h i s o w n a g e a n d s o c i a l s t a t u s w h o h a s d e v e l o p e d a g o o d v o i c e a n d r e t u r n e d t o w o r k . t h e n e w p a t i e n t m u s t c o n s t a n t l y b e r e a s s u r e d t h a t l o s s of s p e e c h is o n l y t e m p o r a r y . 2. patient's approach to his convalescence h o w m u c h t h e p a t i e n t a p p r e c i a t e s h i s p o s t o p e r a t i v e c o n d i t i o n d e p e n d s u p o n h i s a g e , e d u c a t i o n , e x p e r i e n c e , a n d g e n e r a l p e r s o n a l i t y m a k e u p . 3. speech re-education i a m n o t in s y m p a t h y w i t h b e g i n n i n g ins t r u c t i o n p r i o r t o s u r g e r y , b u t i d o r e c o m m e n d t h a t t h e p u p i l b e g i v e n t h e b a s i c f a c t s of h o w e s o p h a g e a l s p e e c h w i l l b e p r o d u c e d . t h i s s h o u l d b e c o n f i n e d t o a c o n s i d e r a t i o n of t h e g e n e r a l c h a r a c t e r i s t i c s of e s o p h a g e a l s p e e c h q u a l i t y of t o n e a n d c o n t r o l of a i r w i t h o u t , a t t h i s t i m e , b u r d e n i n g t h e p u p i l w i t h t h e m e c h a n i c s a n d p r o b l e m s a s s o c i a t e d w i t h i t s p r o d u c t i o n . h e m u s t b e a d v i s e d t h a t , for a t i m e , h e s h o u l d c o m m u n i c a t e b y w r i t i n g a n d in n o c a s e r e s o r t t o w h i s p e r i n g . a l s o , i t s h o u l d b e s t r e s s e d t h a t m u c h of h i s s u c c e s s in a c q u i r i n g h i s n e w v o i c e d e p e n d s a l m o s t e n t i r e l y o n h i s d e t e r m i n a t i o n t o p r a c t i s e r e g u l a r l y . i h a v e f o u n d t h a t t h e e a r l i e r i n s t r u c t i o n is b e g u n f o l l o w i n g s u r g e r y t h e m o r e s a t i s f a c t o r y t h e r e s u l t s . i h a v e f o l l o w e d f o u r r a t h e r s i m p l e s t e p s in d e v e l o p i n g t h i s v o i c e a n d a l w a y s r e f r a i n f r o m d i s c u s s i n g s u r g e r y w i t h t h e p a t i e n t . t h e s e f o u r s t e p s a r e : 1) o p e n m o u t h 2 ) c l o s e m o u t h 3) s w a l l o w a i r ( s a m e a s o n e s w a l l o w s food o r d r i n k ) . 4 ) o p e n m o u t h a t o n c e a n d w i t h l i p s t r y t o s a y " b a . " y o u w i l l n o t e t h a t , i n t h e v e r y first a p p r o a c h t o t h i s p r o g r a m , 1 c a l l a t t e n t i o n t o t h e u s e of 1 journal of the south african logopedic society august, 1963 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) t h e l i p s in f o r m i n g s o u n d . o v e r t h e y e a r s i h a v e u s e d p h o n e t i c s o u n d s . t h i s i b e l i e v e h a s s e v e r a l a d v a n t a g e s t h e p u p i l is n o t c o n f r o n t e d w i t h t r y i n g t o s a y w o r d s w h e n a t t h a t t i m e , h i s m e n t a l r e a c t i o n is t h a t h e c a n n o t s p e a k , a n d s p e e c h is m a d e of w o r d s e i t h e r s i n g l y o r in s e q u e n c e . m a n y of t h e s e p h o n e t i c s o u n d s a r e w o r d s in t h e m s e l v e s , s u c h a s b e , bi, m e , m i , ti, t o m e n t i o n a few. m y e n t i r e a p p r o a c h is g e a r e d t o w h a t t h e p a t i e n t h i m s e l f c a n a c c o m p l i s h a n d d o w e l l . s i n g l e s y l l a b l e s h a v i n g b e e n a r t i c u l a t e d a n d e n u n c i a t e d w e l l , 1 t h e n p r o c e e d t o d o u b l i n g , t r i p l i n g , a n d t h e u s e of m o r e a d v a n c e d r h y t h m s , t h u s e n a b l i n g t h e p u p i l n o t o n l y t o l e a r n t o c o n t r o l t h i s a i r , b u t a t t h e s a m e t i m e g i v i n g h i m m u c h v a r i e t y i n p i t c h . . t h e s e r h y t h m s a r e m o r e f u l l y e x p l a i n e d i n m y m a n u a l " e s o p h a g e a l s p e e c h " a n d t h e r e c o r d i n g w h i c h i h a v e m a d e for h o m e p r a c t i c e . 4. solving the practical problems of living without a larynx t o m a n y p u p i l s t h e d a y s i m m e d i a t e l y foll o w i n g s u r g e r y a r e t h e m o s t f r u s t r a t i n g . m a n y q u e s t i o n s a r i s e i n h i s m i n d s u c h a s t h e c o n t r o l of m u c o u s , a b i l i t y t o b r e a t h e , p r o b l e m s of e a t i n g , d r e s s i n g a n d m a t t e r s of p e r s o n a l h y g i e n e . s o m e of t h e s e c o u l d h a v e b e e n e x p l a i n e d p r i o r t o s u r g e r y , b u t t h e y b e c o m e m o r e r e a l i s t i c o n c e s u r g e r y h a s b e e n p e r f o r m e d . t h o s e c l o s e l y a s s o c i a t e d w i t h t h e p u p i l s h o u l d a p p r o a c h t h e s e p r o b l e m s w i t h a v e r y p o s i t i v e a t t i t u t e , n o t o n e of s y m p a t h y b u t r a t h e r , o n e of c o m p l e t e u n d e r s t a n d i n g . t h e p u p i l s h o u l d b e a s s u r e d t h a t h e w i l l b e a b l e t o e a t a n d d r e s s a s h e d i d p r i o r t o s u r g e r y , t h a t h e c a n s h o w e r , m a k i n g s u r e t h a t t h e s t o m a is c o v e r e d a t all t i m e s . h e r e t h e i m p o r t a n c e of t e a m w o r k w i t h t h e d o c t o r , n u r s e , f a m i l y , s o c i a l w o r k e r s , a n d s p e e c h t h e r a p i s t is of u t m o s t a s s i s t a n c e t o t h e p u p i l . m a n y y e a r s a g o t h e p a t i e n t c o u l d a n t i c i p a t e o n l y l y e a r s of s i l e n c e , b u t t o d a y , w i t h n e w surgijcal t e c h n i q u e s a n d t h e a d v a n c e m e n t in b e t t e r t e a c h i n g m e t h o d s of e s o p h a g e a l s p e e c h , t h e r e is m u c h e n c o u r a g e m e n t for t h e p a t i e n t t o l o o k f o r w a r d t o a n o r m a l a n d v e r y s a t i s f a c t o r y life in t h e f u t u r e . summary t h e s u d d e n l o s s of s p e e c h i n t h e l a r y n g e c t o m e e is a t r a u m a t i c a n d f r i g h t e n i n g e x p e r i e n c e . h e m u s t b e p r e p a r e d b e f o r e t h e o p e r a t i o n for w h a t is t o f o l l o w a n d b e rea s s u r e d t h a t t h e l o s s of s p e e c h is o n l y t e m p o r a r y . p r i o r t o s u r g e r y t h e p a t i e n t s h o u l d b e g i v e n t h e b a s i c f a c t s of h o w e s o p h o g e a l s p e e c h w i l l b e p r o d u c e d . h e m u s t b e a d v i s e d t h a t i m m e d i a t e l y a f t e r t h e o p e r a t i o n h e s h o u l d c o m m u n i c a t e i n w r i t i n g , b u t o n n o a c c o u n t r e s o r t t o w h i s p e r e d s p e e c h . t h e r a p y f o l l o w s f o u r r a t h e r s i m p l e s t e p s : (i) o p e n m o u t h ; (ii) c l o s e m o u t h ; (iii) s w a l l o w a i r ( s a m e a s o n e s w a l l o w s food o r d r i n k ) ; ( i v ) o p e n m o u t h a t o n c e a n d w i t h l i p s t r y t o s a y " b a " . p h o n e t i c s o u n d s a r e u s e d in p r e f e r e n c e t o w o r d s , w h i c h m a y , i n t h e e a r l y s t a g e s , h a v e a c q u i r e d n e g a t i v e a s p e c t s for t h e p a t i e n t . a s t h e r a p y p r o g r e s s e s , m o r e a d v a n c e d s y l l a b l e s a n d r h y t h m s a r e u s e d . p e r s o n a l p r o b l e m s w i t h r e g a r d t o a d j u s t m e n t s h o u l d b e d e a l t w i t h a s t h e y a r i s e . opsomming d i e s k i e l i k e v e r l i e s v a n s p r a a k is v i r d i e l a r i n g e k t o m i e ' n t r o u m a t i e s e e n v r e e s a a n j a e n d e o n d e r v i n d i n g . d i e p a s i e n t m o e t d u s v o o r d i e o p e r a s i e v o o r b e r e i w o r d v i r w a t g a a n v o l g e n h y m o e t g e r u s g e s t e l w o r d d a t d i e s p r a a k v e r l i e s n e t t y d e l i k s a l w e e s . v o o r s j i r u r g i e s e b e h a n d e l i n g m o e t a a n d i e p a s i e n t a l d i e b a s i e s e feite i n v e r b a n d m e t e s o f a g e a l e s p r a a k e n d i e p r o d u k s i e d a a r v a n g e g e e w o r d . h y b e h o o r t a a n g e r a a i t e w o r d o m d i r e k n a d i e o p e r a s i e d e u r m i d d e l v a n s k r i f t e k o m m u n i k e e r e n n o o i t g e f l u i s t e r d e s p r a a k t e g e b r u i k n i e . t e r a p i e w o r d in 4 e e n v o u d i g e s t a p p e v e r d e e l : (i) m a a k m o n d o o p ; (ii) m a a k m o n d t o e ; (iii) s l u k l u g ( s o o s k o s e n v l o e i s t o w w e g e s l u k w o r d ) ; (iv) m a a k m o n d o o p e n p r o b e e r d a d e l i k m e t d i e l i p p e , , b a " s6. f o n e t i e s e k l a n k e w o r d v e r k i e s l i k g e b r u i k b o w o o r d e , o m d a t w o o r d e a a n d i e b e g i n ' n n e g a t i e w e r e a k s i e b y d i e p a s i e n t k a n o n t l o k . n a m a t e d i e p a t i e n t m e t t e r a p i e v o r d e r , w o r d m e e r g e v o r d e r d e l e t t e r g r e p e g e b r u i k . p e r s o o n l i k h e i d s p r o b l e r n e t e n o p s i g t e v a n a a n p a s s i n g s m o e t h a n t e e r w o r d w a n n e e r h u l l e t e v o o r s k y n k o m . 1 journal of the south african logopedic society 19 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) j o u r n a l of t h e s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c society editorial l o g o p e d i c s . like all new sciences, is still having an uphill sfruagle in this country. both the medical a n d lay public h a v e yet to learn that the h a n d i c a p p e d in speech can derive great benefit from trained workers in th« held, a n d that s p e e c h therapy is desirable and a v a i l a b l e . ' the university of the v/itwatersrand has pioneered ihe training of speech therapists a n d h a s maintained a high standard of a c a d e m i c achievement c o m p a r a b l e with most lcgopedic centres in the world. of the four provinces in south africa, the transvaal h a s t a k e r the lead m providing s p e e c h therapy services in hospitals, w h e r e a s in the c a p e there a r e more school posts than in a n y other province. it is honed that those in authority will s e e the need for establishing similar facilities in au parts of the country. in practice, the furtherance of speech therapy deoends on public interest a n d demand. likewise, the creation of posts must b e determined by th~ number of a v a i l a b l e speech therapists. few matriculants in this country o r a w a r e of s p e e c h therapy a s a career, and a s a result, there a r e insufficient g r a d u a t e s to meet the potential demand. the executive committee of the south african lcgopedic society recognizes the n e e d to e n c o u r a g e young men and women to train as *p»ech therapists, a n d h a s a l r e a d y taken steps in this direction. in addition a programme is being worked out to make speech therapy better known to the medical a n d auxiliary professions. members of the south african lcgopedic s o c i e y c a n therefore look forward with confidence to the future of speech therapy in this country. vacancy for speech therapist the pretoria school for cerebral palsied children, p.o. box 1551, pretoria, telephone 20626, has 1 vacancy for speech therapist. enquiries: principal's office. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for a group of tongue thrust swallowers and their mothers nigel bligh b.a. log. (rand), m.a. (rand) department of speech pathology & audiology, # university of the witwatersrand, johannesburg summary a brief description of tongue thrusting and a s s o c i a t e d problems i s given. the therapy programme for a group of t o n g u e thrusting chddren and their mothers is d e s c r i b e d traditional swallow retraining was used w i t h the group of children in c o n j u n c t i o n w i t h exercises for strengthening and increasing oro-sensory awareness o f t o n g u e and lips. a therapist guided the mothers discussions while t h e y observed therapy. results indicated that therapy was successful. the c o n c l u s i o n s suggest further avenues of research. opsomming 'n kort beskrywing van infantiele slukpatroon en verwante p r o b l e m e w o r d gegee. die terapieprogram vir 'n groep kinders m e t infantiele slukpatroon en hul moeders word beskryf. tradisionele slukheropleiding is gebruik m e t hierdie groep kinders tesame met oefeninge vir die versterking en verhoging van orosensoriese b e w u s t h e i d van die t o n g en lippe. 'n terapeut h e t die moeders se besprekings gelei terwyl hulle terapie waargen e e m het. resultate het daarop gedui dat terapie suksesvol was. die gevolgtrekkings b i e d verdere kidrade vir navorsing. one of the perennial problems the speech therapist shares with the orthodontist is the tongue thruster. on the surface this appears a simple difficulty to correct. it is however, extraordinarily resistant to therapy. ihis article will discuss some relevant problems and a suggested therapy programme. symptoms a n d problems a s s o c i a t e d with t o n g u e t h r u s t incorrect swallow pattern the classification of simple, complex and retained infantile swallow3, will be disregarded as it is irrelevant to the rehabilitation programme. the main identifying feature is a forward movement of the tongue, generally against or between the teeth, during swallowing, with deviant usage of severa muscle groups, e.g. supra and infrahyoid, massefer, temporalis, and orbicularis oris. * now at 626 e. minnehaha parkway, minneapolis, minn. 55417, usa. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for a group of t o n g u e thrust swallowers and their mothers movement of the lips and contraction of the muscles of mastication are often evident during swallowing as a.lip-seal is used instead of a tongue/palate seal.18 the individual resists attempts to part the lips during a swallow. effect on orofacial structure as a result of regular incorrect pressure of the tongue on the teeth3 and the added pressure of the muscles of deglutition, mandibular growth and natural dental alignment may be affected.15 top and occasionally, bottom teeth may protrude. irregular spacing is not uncommon. the type of dental involvement will depend on the nature of the incorrect swallow. the malocclusion may prevent a natural relaxed position of the mouth at rest, so that the lips will not exert the optimal pressure necessary for facial development. the loss of regular pressure may lead to a high vaulted palate.1 1>1 5 there appears to be loss of tone and strength in the lips and tongue.1-1 0* individuals who swallow incorrectly seem to have excessive muscle activity and develop muscle patterns that are not as clearcut or definite as those of normal swallowers.10 medical history tongue thrusters often have a history of tonsil, adenoid and respiratory involvement, which can lead to other problems associated with tongue thrust, 1 7 i.e. chronic mouth breathing, either from necessity or habit. this in turn causes a tongue rest position with tongue tip away from the alveolar ridge.3 sucking problems there is not infrequently a problem of sucking as a baby. it has been suggested that nipples that allow the milk to flow too freely lead to incorrect swallowing.15 thumb and finger sucking is common, accentuating dental misalignment9, and incorrect swallow patterns.3 speech a study by fletcher et al4 found a relationship between tongue thrust swallowing pattern and sibilant distortion. although not automatically involved, typically speech has tip-dental production of tip-alveolar sounds.3·1 0 speech will often be accoustically acceptable but visually unacceptable. therapy therapy for tongue thrust given either by the orthodontist or speech therapist, is frequently described in the l i t e r a t u r e . 3 · 6 · 1 0 · 1 6 ' 1 8 it involves basically either the re-training of the musculature involved in swallowing, or the use of a habit-breaker appliance. results are often equivocal. one of the main difficulties would appear to be the evocation of adequate motivation to form new h a b i t s . 6 ' 1 5 * confirmed in personal communication with dr c. overstake. .. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en cehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 2 nigel bligh additional unverified observations (i) there is an impression of lowered kinesthetic and orosensory awareness, e.g. tolerance to extremes of temperature, inability to move the tongue accurately on demand1 8, and difficulty in moving the tongue without concomitant lip and jaw movements. (ii) the difficulty seems to be commoner amongst girls than boys. of sixteen tongue thrusters receiving therapy at the speech & hearing clinic, university of the witwatersrand, five were boys. in a study by mims et al9 discussing the relationships between thumb sucking and open-bite malocclusions, there were 24 boys and 52 girls. (iii) there seems to be a familial tendency to tongue thrust. 1 3 method the aims of the study were (a) to investigate an approach to therapy for tongue thrusters, (b) to investigate certain aspects of group therapy, i.e. group size, age range and variation in therapist, and (c) to explore a method of parent counselling. group t h e r a p y programme subjects there were six girls and three boys ranging in age from four and a half to eleven years. the four and a half-year old was taken despite evidence in the literature14 that tongue thrusting is a normal developmental stage because his. teeth were already being pushed out of alignment and there was a definite familial tendency. this familial tendency was noted in the two sets of siblings in-ihe group and two of the other children. five of the children were being seen for individual articulation therapy once a week, in addition to the weekly group therapy for tongue thrust. because it was a group project, it was unnecessary to eliminate those children who did not have speech defects purely because this was a speech clinic.17 it will be noted that both the size of the group and the age range, are in conflict with most of the recommendations in the literature 8 / therapists / one graduate therapist and six speech and hearing therapy students at the university of the witwatersrand participated in the weekly group therapy. the student therapists were involved in individual articulation therapy for tongue thrusters on a second afternoon. initially the graduate therapist took the group with the students observing. therapists then alternated as group leader with the others observing and later participating. after each session there was a seminar to discuss the therapy'administered and plan the following week's session. the effects on the group of alternating therapists were noted throughout. journal of he south african spch a hearing association, vol. 19, dcmbr 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for a group of t o n g u e thrust swallowers and their mothers 3 3 therapy aims 1. to eliminate any causal or maintaining factors of tongue thrust. 2. to explain to the children why they were coming for therapy and to generate motivation. 3. to correct the swallow pattern. 4. to correct the rest position of tongue and lips. 5. to increase proprioception, kinesthesia, tone and strength of tongue and lips. therapy procedures throughout therapy all drill work and explanations were presented in a suitably motivating and understandable fashion. 1. all children were under observation by orthodontists. four of them had been referred for therapy before orthodontics would be attempted. three children were encouraged to stop sucking their thumbs and one had his tonsils and adenoids removed. 2. (a) the children learnt that they were exerting lvilbs of thrust on their teeth 2 000 times a day, i.e. each time they swallowed3'11 (ehrlich3mentions 6 lbs). the children were encouraged to discuss what effect this would have on their looks. (b) they learnt the hygienic reasons for breathing through their noses rather than through their mouths. (c) they were taught that with better control and proprioceptive awareness of their lips and tongues they would be better able to place them where they wanted to and keep them there. 3. the following rules were learnt (overstake10):(a) put your teeth and lips lightly together. (b) place the tip of your tongue on the alveolar ridge. (c) suck the food onto your tongue. (d) then push your tongue onto your palate and swallow. (e) your tongue must not move forward. 4. there were intermittent reminders for the children to "look nice and feel nice", i.e. to have their mouths shut and tongue tips resting lightly on the alveolar ridges. 5. tongue and lip therapy was divided into: (a) movement lips were spread and pursed as in an exaggerated "oo-ee". tongue was moved sideways, in and out, up and down. instructions were given to place the tongue in specific positions. the main aim was to get independent tongue movement, i.e. without concomitant lip and jaw movement and to achieve better awareness of tongue position in the mouth. (b) icing is a technique advocated by physiotherapists and speech therapists working with cerebral palsied children. used for any length of time ice acts as an anaesthetic. for shorter periods it seems in most cases to tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 4 nigel bligh encourage relaxation, 7 , 1 2 increases the blood supply to the surface areas and encourages increased proprioceptive awareness. empirical use indicates that it is a useful technique to use in articulation therapy. with this group it was used in conjunction with resistance exercises. (c) resistance is a technique for strengthening and improving coordination of muscles.7 the amount of resistance offered must never be greater than the muscle strength, i.e. the individual must always be able to complete the movement. resistance should obviously increase as the muscle gets stronger.5·12 the children's tongues or lips were iced and the following resistance exercises were then done:-* i) as the child protruded the tongue the tip was gently resisted by the therapist or by another child holding a wooden spatula. ii) the child moved the tongue towards one side while movement was resisted with the spatula from the other. iii) the child elevated or depressed the tongue tip with resistance from the other direction. this generally required two spatulas, one above the tongue to aid movement, and one below to resist, and vice versa. assistance was necessary for those children who had difficulty elevating or depressing the tongue. iv) lip movement was gently resisted by the therapists' fingers held at each corner of the mouth, as the child spread or pursed his lips. at the end of each session the children were given a home programme to encourage carry-over of all therapy activities. a resistance technique that may be worth investigating, especially for swallow retraining, is the use of a v2" cube of sponge rubber securely attached to a cord that the therapist pulls to resist the tongue motion while swallowing.7 c o u n s e l l i n g programme six mothers were counselled regularly and one irregularly. the aims were to explain the rationale behind therapy, and to motivate parents to carry on with the programme at home. initially the mothers were counselled briefly and given the week's home programme after the group therapy by the graduate therapist with the students observing. subsequently the mothers observed the entire therapy session through a one-way mirror with therapists taking it in turn to guide observation and discussion.. the children were all aware of the fact that their mothers were observing. the discussion revolved around the nature of tongue thrust and the associated problems, what therapy was aiming at and the difficulties inherent in treating the problem. as therapy progressed, discussion of different approaches, techniques and reactions of the various children were encouraged. * adapted from suggestions b y miss j . blair, head of the d e p t . of physiotherapy, university of the witwatersrand. journal of he south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for a group of t o n g u e t h r u s t swallowers and their mothers 35 results and discussion by the very nature of the disorder and its prognosis, tongue thrust therapy has a tendency to be both dull and rather discouraging.3 it is difficult to alter oral patterns that are so closely related to reflexes.18 tongue thrust therapy in therapy all children showed a definite improvement in the manner of swallowing, and the elimination of concomitant problems, i.e. mouth breathing, thumb sucking, etc. there was a consistent improvement in articulation. the mothers reported an increased awareness of the problem and its associated difficulties. all reported good carry-over during therapy. there was a definite but incomplete regression when regular therapy stopped, due to university examinations. for this reason all the children were referred to the dental clinic of the university of the witwatersrand to be fitted with habit breaker plates. dental impressions and x-rays were taken and the children were given simple plates with exaggerated rugae to remind them of the correct tongue tip position. these were to be worn during the break from therapy. in view of the therapy the children had had, it was felt unnecessary to make use of any type of grid to prevent forward movement of the t o n g u e . 1 3 ' 1 5 during the therapist discussions, it was suggested that the use of straws for drinking11 did not assist the tongue in the correct movement for swallowing, but in fact, tended to encourage both a forward tongue movement and an excess of movement in the orbicularis oris. it was therefore discontinued as a therapy tool. group therapy the children enjoyed therapy and co-operated well. it was found that activities that fostered movement and laughter were more successful than others and resulted in better group cohesion. the large number in the group proved entirely satisfactory and manageable and made several of the activities more feasible. even though therapy was geared to a very physical level, personality changes were evident, e.g. the shyer children became more open and the rather sophisticated child became more pleasingly child-like. one of the older girls, who was resistant to all previous attempts to deal with her problem benefitted from the opportunity to share a common experience with the others. not only did she see that there were several other children in the same position, but she also soon became a leader in the group. the age range proved to be an advantage in that the older children were not only encouraged to assist the younger ones, but in fact did a great deal to help them. the potential danger that the older children would dominate the younger8 was not realised. it was however, evident that the group had to be confined to primary school children. because of the disparity of abilities, it was found to be important to eliminate any element of competition and to tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en ehoorheekunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) nigel bligh 3 6 n n r e n t r a t e on co-operation.2 this meant that the therapist had to be very p e r c e p t i v e to the reactions of the children both to each other and to their own achievements. the therapy programme was aimed mainly at providing guidance for the home programmes, i.e. therapy was not merely thirty minutes of drill to be forgotten until the next week. thus all homework activities grew out of the preceding session or led logically towards the next. it did not seem to be important to the children that the therapist varied from session to session. it was almost as though the important inter-relationships were inter-child relationships and the therapist was there mainly to guide activities. the therapists found therapy entertaining and challenging. the discussions after each session ensured continuity and resulted in some novel and stimulating ideas and an ever improving therapeutic approach. parent counselling the parents were an integral part of the therapy programme. they knew exactly what their children were doing in therapy and why they were doing it. they became interested in the whole programme and accepted the fact that prognosis was not very good. they were receptive to any new approaches that were tried. the therapists felt that the interest shown by the mothers contributed to the excellent attendance, the fact that homework was always done and to the general positive attitude to therapy. additional positive effects of this form of counselling were: (a) the mothers saw how the therapists handled the children. (b) parents often spontaneously noted factors about their children that led to greater objectivity and greater reception to therapist suggestions. (c) the group discussion often revolved around an incident in therapy that did not necessarily involve tongue thrusting but was relevant to the child. for optimal effectiveness, this type of approach required two therapists one with the children and one guiding the parent discussions. conclusions it was felt generally that this approach to therapy with tongue thrusters was successful in that both mothers and children were motivated to work on the problem. overall results were most encouraging. ideas were generated that might lead to better carry-over and, finally, total rehabilitation. these included: 1 1. planning the programme so that therapy could gradually be terminated to ensure complete rehabilitation. journal of the south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) therapy for a group of t o n g u e thrust swallowers and their mothers 2. using dental study models and x-rays before and after therapy not only as an objective measure of progress but also as a motivating force and an excellent means of explaining the problem. 3. investigating different types of habit breakers and using them in conjunction with the therapy programme. 4. developing the home programme further, extending this to a more active participation by the teacher and the group at the school. 5. investigating the use of aids such as straws, in correcting swallowing patterns. the group therapy techniques were successful and could be applied with advantage to other problems. in fact it would be interesting to investigate whether factors such as large group size, identification with one therapist, etc., would be as apparently insignificant with a group of children whose problem was more psychological. general investigations into the factors contributing towards general cohesion in group therapy for speech defectives may well be rewarding. the method of counselling mothers would probably be excellent for other difficulties especially where a greater understanding of therapeutic approach and handling of the children is required. acknowledgement i would like to thank the following students in the department of speech pathology and audiology, university of the witwatersrand for their co-operation, enthusiasm and suggestions: misses f. janks, c. linton, n. moss, y. sarfin, b. shakinovsky and m. weinberg. my colleague, mrs m. noach, gave many suggestions, especially with regard to the habit breaker plate. references 1. carrell, j.a. (1968) disorders of articulation. foundation of speech pathology series. prentice hall. 2. deutsch, m. (1954) the effects of co-operation and competition upon group process in d. cartwright and a. zender, eds. group dynamics research and theory. tavistock publications ltd. 3. ehrlich, ann beard (1970) training therapists for tongue thriist correction. charles c. thomas. 4. fletcher, s.g., casteel, r.l. and bradley, d.p. (1961) tongue thrust swallow, speech articulation and age./. speech hearing dis., 26, 201-208. tydskrif van die suid-afrikaanse vereniging vir spraak en gehoorheelkunde, vol. 19, desember 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 3 8 nigel bligh 5. gardiner, m.d. (1935) the principals of exercise therapy. g. bell and sons limited. 6. hanson, m.l. (1967) some suggestions for more effective therapy for tongue thrust./. speech and hearingdis., 32, 75-79. 7. knott, m. and voss, d.e. (1968) proprioceptive neuromuscular facilitation patterns and techniques, 2nd ed. harper & row. 8. macdonald, e.t. (1967) articulation testing and treatment: a sensory motor approach, stanwix house, inc., pittsburgh. 9. mims, h.a., kolas, c. and williams, r. (1966) lisping and persistent thumb-sucking among children with open bite malocclusions. j. speech hearing dis., 31,176-178. 10. overstake, c.p. (1970) an investigation of tongue thrust swallowing and the functional relationship of deviant swallowing, orthodontic problems and speech defects. ash a national convention. 11. rogers, j.h. (1961) swallowing patterns of a normal population sample compared to those of patients from an orthodontic practice. amer. j. ortho., 47, 674-689. 12. rood, m.s. (1956) neurophysiological mechanisms utilised in treatment of neuromuscular dysfunction. amer. j. ofocc. therapy, 4,no.2, pp 220 et seq. 13. salzman, j.a. (1971) training therapists for tongue thrust correction. book review. amer. j. ortho., 59, p.411. 14. shelton, r.l. (1963) therapeutic exercises and speech pathology. asha, 5, 855-859. 15. straub, w.j. (1961) malfunction of the tongue part ii. the abnormal swallowing habit. its causes, effects and results in relationship to orthodontic treatment and speech therapy. amer. j. orth., 47, 596-617. 16. straub, w.j. (1962) malfunction of the tongue part iii. amer. j. orth., 48,486-503. 17. subtelny, j. daniel and subtelny, joanne e. (1962) malocclusion, speech & deglutition. amer. j. orth., 48, 685-697. 18. wood, j.m. (1971) tongue thrusting, some clinical observations. j. speech hearing dis., 36, 82-89. journal of i he south african speech and hearing association, vol. 19, december 1972 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 22 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 maximising health literacy and client recall of clinical information: an exploratory study of clients and speech language pathologists friderike schmidt von wühlisch michelle pascoe division of communication sciences and disorders, faculty of health sciences, university of cape town correspondence to: f schmidt von wühlisch (friderike.svw@gmail.com) clients in the health care sector frequently struggle to understand and remember details of clinical information and reasons underlying their treatment. as a result, they often do not adhere to clinical instructions and recommendations, which contributes to a reduction in the effectiveness and efficiency of health care interventions (kessels, 2003). therefore, to support clients in managing their own health, clients need to be provided with a documented record of client-specific clinical information (osborne, 2008a & 2008b; santo, laizner & shohet, 2005). this poses specific challenges for people who have low literacy skills as they have fewer means to either improve their health literacy (e.g. through the internet) or review health advice because they cannot read written materials. in the field of speech-language pathology, little research has been conducted with regard to health literacy and client recall. however, speech-language pathologists (slps) frequently provide vast amounts of information that clients need to understand, apply and review in order to manage their (or their child’s) health. this study responds to the need to investigate conventional and unconventional strategies of maximising health literacy and client recall post consultation. health literacy: a background health literacy is defined as ‘the degree to which individuals have the capacity to obtain, process, and understand basic health information and the services needed to make appropriate health decisions’ (institute of medicine (iom), 2004, p. 1). clients with low health and general literacy skills may have poorer health, higher expenses for health care, a higher rate of hospitalisations, lower self-efficacy for preventive care practices and compliance to treatment regimens (ross, 2007). this is true regardless of socio-economic status. people can have years of education and have high functional literacy (reading, writing and numeracy skills), but their health literacy skills can still be limited (williams, baker, parker & nurss, 1998). health literacy skills are not bound to race or age, even though the interagency coordinating council (2002) stated that 80% of the population aged 60 years and older in the usa alone have insufficient health literacy skills. health literacy problems have grown as clinicians and health care system providers expect clients to assume more responsibility for their care at a time when the health system is progressively more fragmented, specialised, complex and technologically sophisticated. the complexity of written and verbal health information and the shortage of health information in languages other than english make it difficult for individuals with low general literacy skills and language differences to communicate effectively in health care (zagaria, 2006). a majority of printed health education materials, regardless of their topic, require relatively high literacy skills that may not exist among many of their target population (gal & prigat, 2005). in the health care sector, and especially in speech-language pathology, where written information is frequently provided, this may lead to extreme frustration on the part of the client. people with speech, language, visual, hearing and intellectual disabilities experience even greater challenges when they need to apply high-level health literacy skills (hester & stevens-ratchford, 2009). clinicians often overestimate their clients’ literacy skills because many clients develop compensatory behaviours. physical characteristics, such as appearance or the clients’ ability to communicate well, are often inaccurate indicators of their literacy level. even when clinicians have worked with clients for years, they are frequently surprised at the poor reading skills of some of their most articulate clients (kelly & haidet, 2007). while some clinicians only provide verbal information to clients, this approach has the potential to disempower clients, as they are unable to refer back to information when they are at home or may not remember what has been discussed. numerous studies on health literacy have been published in various fields, including nursing, paediatrics, psychology, oncology, hiv/aids and pharmaceutics. topics range from ways to improve health literacy to issues around client-provider communication (bellardie & harris, 2008; chang & kelly, 2007; sanders & brosco, 2005). the limited research carried out in speech-language pathology and audiology is striking given that the iom (2004) identified speaking and listening as areas in need of improvement in health literacy research and intervention. two studies relevant to these professions were identified, which focused on the readability of educational materials for clients with cleft lip and/or palate and their families, as well as the effects of reader and text variables on understanding of health information in adults (harris, fleming & mcdougall, 2003; kahn & pannbacker, 2000). these studies suggest that individuals with communication difficulties and their carers are at an even greater risk of not understanding written materials. there is currently no consensus among health professionals and researchers about which interventions should be incorporated into clinical practice. abstract limited research has been carried out in the field of speech-language pathology with regard to ways of maximising health literacy and client recall. however, speech-language pathologists (slps) frequently provide vast amounts of information that clients need to understand, apply and review in order to manage their (or their child’s) health. this exploratory study aimed to contribute information about ways in which slps can overcome low health literacy and poor client recall so that treatment effectiveness is improved. a case-study design was used with specific focus on four clients receiving treatment for dysphagia, voice disorders (including laryngectomies) and cleft lip and/or palate management in cape town. strategies which may be able to maximise health literacy and client recall of clinical information were trialled and evaluated by clients and their slps, using semi-structured interviews. the researchers proposed a combination of high-tech strategies which assisted in all the cases. no single solution or universal tool was found that would be appropriate for all. there is a need to evaluate the long-term effectiveness of the combined strategies across a wider population, at different stages of rehabilitation and in diverse contexts. implications and suggestions for future related research are presented. keywords: adherence, effectiveness, health literacy, intervention, recall vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 23 health literacy and client recall regardless of the guidelines that have been provided to improve health literacy, the issue of client recall of health care instructions and its effects on the actual outcomes of management is still present. client recall: a background an important aspect in providing health information to clients, in addition to issues of health literacy, is that of clients’ recall of information and instructions after consultations. recall (both short and long-term retention) of health information and recommendations are prerequisites for adherence to treatment (kessels, 2003). this is an important point to consider in light of the widely held assumption that only good health literacy skills are responsible for an increase in adherence to recommendations. it is apparent that the speechlanguage pathology and audiology professions pay minimal attention to issues around clients’ memory of health information, even though retention of vast amounts of important information (i.e. diagnosis and recommendations) is critical to treatment outcomes. some clients find little logic in what happens during and after consultations. this is because they face the stress of a medical crisis, often without the presence of a significant other person or while in great pain, confusion or depression (rao, 2007). forty to 80% of medical information presented by clinicians is forgotten immediately and nearly half of the information that is actually remembered is incorrect (kessels, 2003). in light of the above, parkin and skinner (2003) found that even when clinicians assume that their clients would recall the health information that was provided, this might not necessarily be the case. there are various explanations for clients’ tendency to forget clinical information. with reference to the clinician: complex language and the provision of too much and disorganised information can make it difficult for a client to recall information. with reference to the client: level of education, stress, anxiety and state of health at the time of consultation; increasing age; perceived unimportance and complexity of information; differences in role expectations regarding rights, duties and responsibilities; and clients ‘switching off ’ as a result of feeling overwhelmed by the emotional impact of the information that they receive may affect recall (north, cornbleet, knowles & leonard, 1992). it would seem to be important to address recall of clinical information as well as health-literacy skills during consultations with all clients and especially with the elderly, considering that the general ageing process can create difficulty in recalling clinical information (kessels, 2003). further issues around information exchange and compliance: the south african context south african slps meet clients representing many different language and dialectal groups, and with a range of literacy abilities. these variables can have an influence on the outcomes of health care. south african clients’ cultural backgrounds also play an important role in the utilisation of health care (kagee, 2004). knowledge of clients’ cultural backgrounds is a vital characteristic for any health professional working in the south african context, e.g. some families may prefer traditional health and healing practices to westernised therapy processes (zhang & bennet, 2001). if clients choose not to adhere to treatment prescribed table i. strategies and their advantages/disadvantages in intervention strategy advantages disadvantages written materials allows recall and revision of important clinical information only for people who have good literacy skills easy to file, store and copy cultural mismatch: some cultures heavily favour oral language, appropriate for speedy review and reference and written materials are less likely to be used as a means for accessing information illustrations aid understanding of complex information a certain level of visual literacy is required very useful for clients with low literacy skills some information is too abstract and complex to be depicted looks interesting and eye-catching in illustrations telephonic contact human contact which can be speedily accessed: best at times of crisis clients may not have a telephone of their own (or no airtime) useful for reminder calls clients may not be willing to call their clinician builds a relationship between slp and client client and clinician need to share a language mobile phones widely available audio-visual materials does not require literacy skills clients need access to equipment human contact: listening to a voice is reassuring recording consultations might inhibit open discussions durable and relatively inexpensive could be translated into any language minimally adds to clinician’s workload can be shared with other significant persons sms cellphones are widely used (over 70% of south africans make use of clients need to have good literacy skills and be familiar with the mobile phones) (internet world stats, 2010) technology best when recommendations are simple and can be given in point form might be less used by older clients who are not used to this form a tool for quick reminders of communication by using the ‘please call me’ function, clients can get into telephonic need for good manual dexterity contact with the slp at no cost not for life-threatening situations as there is too much room clients use from the privacy of their own home and caregivers do not for misunderstandings need to leave the patient unaccompanied restrictive (often too much information that needs to be conveyed) can be time-consuming cannot provide practical demonstrations or immediate feedback reminders on mobile serves best as a post-consultation reminder of important points need for good manual dexterity phones people carry mobile phones with them at all times so reminders restrictive (often too much information that needs to be would be immediate conveyed) not all mobile phones have this function additional time is needed to type reminders into mobile phones all mobile phones operate differently so slps need to get accustomed to them first personalised clinical tailored to the unique needs, interests and concerns of the individual clinicians find it time-consuming to prepare information more effective in assisting clients adhere to recommendations than general information materials unnecessary information is eliminated information is more relevant to the individual 24 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 health literacy and client recall by an slp because of their cultural beliefs, they might be perceived by the clinician as non-compliant. for example, slps may advise parents to stimulate their children’s speech and language from an early age, but in some cultures it is not customary to speak to children directly until they reach a certain age (marfo, 1993). what may also increase the perception of non-compliance, especially in the south african population, is that there is a culturally determined tendency to accept information non-interactively, without objections or questioning, even when the information may not be entirely understood (marfo, 1993). this characteristic may be explained by tactics of saving face, different child-rearing practices, perceived unequal power relations between clients and professionals, short duration of consultations and a hesitancy to be open with authoritarian figures such as medical doctors (cilliers, 2005). clients might also prefer a rather ‘paternalistic style’ of client-provider relationships in consultations which means that the health professional is primarily made responsible for the treatment and well-being of the client. however, clients’ choice of a more passive role in consultations seems to be reasonable given that they do not have the medical resources or knowledge to reach treatment decisions or the authority to implement them. regardless of how this ‘passivity’ or ‘noninteractivity’ is explained, it raises the prospect of reducing clients’ roles in decision making, as well as their success in managing their own health. south africa is a linguistically diverse country where 11 official languages are spoken (big media publishers, 2007). nevertheless, south african public health care is still characterised by a large language divide between clients and clinicians. isizulu and isixhosa have the highest number of first-language speakers in the country, but the majority of south african health professionals are proficient only in english and/ or afrikaans (schwartz, 2004). according to penn (2007), the majority of health interactions in south africa are mediated by a third party, and more than 80% of these interactions between clients, a third party and health professionals take place across linguistic and cultural barriers. where a language mismatch exists between client and clinician, the presence of a properly trained interpreter can be invaluable. when cross-cultural communication is improved, it may lead to increased client involvement in care, better adherence to treatment regimens, a higher quality of care, and better overall health outcomes (cooperpatrick, gallo, gonzales, thi vu, powe, nelson & ford, 1999). in south africa, more than 9 million adults are illiterate (unesco institute for lifelong learning, 2010). furthermore, 2.9 4.2 million people, i.e. 1 in every 5 south africans over the age of 20 years, have not received formal education (nelson mandela foundation, 2004). even though countless south africans can speak several languages, their literacy skills in all of these may be limited. many older generations of people who speak african languages and were educated under the apartheid system, as well as many of the younger generations – as a result of the sudden switch to english as the language of learning and teaching (lolt) in grade 4 – cannot read and write in their mother tongue. additionally, their literacy skills in english and afrikaans, which were the only languages allowed for education during the apartheid era (before the riots in 1976), are not sufficient because of inadequacies in the education that was provided under apartheid (kagee, 2004). today, despite the efforts of the state and non-government organisations (ngos), there are still various challenges in the provision of literacy classes that have to be solved (pretorius, 2004). this means that even when written materials are translated, they may be of little assistance to clients with the above profile. long waiting times at clinics or lengthy time lapses between appointments also affect compliance (mcdonald, garg & haynes, 2002). furthermore, clients typically define adherence in terms of their view of good health and they only seek treatment approaches that are (in their view) manageable, tolerable and effective. many clients only consider medication rather than behavioural regimens to be treatments (kagee, 2004). this is an important factor to consider in light of the nature of slp interventions, which typically require behavioural changes and adherence to therapeutic strategies. poverty is another variable that affects compliance (kagee, 2004). reduced self-efficacy and the demands and stresses of daily survival, work and family life may contribute to the lack of acknowledgement of the importance of complying with treatment regimens. practical solutions for improving health literacy, client recall and compliance post consultation clients often forget or misunderstand information if clinicians do not provide additional materials to ensure that when they leave the clinical setting, the information they have is clear, accurate, complete and available for review and discussion with other professionals or family members (kessels, 2003). there are various studies that tested different interventions (alone and in combination) to improve compliance. these explored, for example, the benefits and concerns related to pictographs (penn, frankel, watermeyer & muller, 2009), video/audio recordings (rao, 2007), short message service (sms/ text messages) (international institute for communication and development, 2006), telephonic contact/follow-up (osborne, 2008a), and personalised clinical information (osborne, 2008b). almost all the interventions that were found to be effective in long-term health care were complex and included combinations of aspects such as more convenient health care, ongoing counselling, and continual reinforcement of recommendations. the complexity of the problem of client non-adherence suggests that no single solution is likely to be helpful, and strategies are more functional when combined (kagee, 2004). table i provides an outline of the strategies and their advantages/disadvantages in intervention. some of the strategies presented in table i are ‘high tech’ (i.e. highly advanced and specialised technology is used) and some are ‘low tech’ (i.e. less advanced technology is involved). while most of the above information aids could be used effectively, not all of them are appropriate for people with complex difficulties, low literacy skills and financial/other resources who live in remote/rural areas. clients with low health/general literacy skills would typically have fewer means to improve their health literacy skills or review health advice because they cannot effectively access health care materials (ross, 2007). clients who live in remote areas cannot attend for frequent therapy – which would provide them with a chance to review information – and some clients are more likely to experience difficulties with compliance as a result of poverty. however, examples of strategies which could support these clients in reviewing information, as well as being in contact with their slp post consultation, are audiovisual materials, telephonic contact and sms/text messages. in the presence of limited financial and other resources in the south african health sector, it is important that issues around client adherence are fully understood and compliance is improved in order to enhance the effectiveness of treatments. with more clients having to take charge of their own health management as a result of financial constraints and (contact) time restrictions with health professionals, there is a dire need to maximise compliance (mcdonald et al., 2002). clients need the opportunity to be in contact with their slp and get feedback when they experience difficulties in treatment. when such difficulties are, for example, associated with dysphagia, potentially severe consequences could result. health professionals and other stakeholders need to commit to being innovative and empowering their clients to access and use health information through culturally appropriate, cost-effective and user-friendly strategies (parker, ratzan & lurie, 2003). from an ethical point of view, slps might be seen to run the risk of overlooking the principles of non-maleficence and beneficence, if the profession does not invest in means to maximise health literacy and client recall of clinical information in populations that are ‘high risk,’ i.e. groups of individuals who are negatively affected by one or more of the many variables that influence health literacy, client recall and general compliance with treatment. vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 25 health literacy and client recall method aims the aims of this study were to: (i) trial technology/strategies which may be able to maximise health literacy and client recall of clinical information; and (ii) evaluate the outcomes of the technology/strategies employed from both the slp’s and the client/caregiver’s perspective. research design a single-subject (case-study) design was used with four cases. traditionally, the focus of single-subject designs is the use of systematic methods for applying interventions and documenting their effects repeatedly in single individuals over a longer period of time (maxwell & satake, 2006). this study only investigated individuals at the first stages of their treatment. this type of design was used nonetheless because it allowed for richness of data in four individual cases. outcomes of intervention were evaluated by means of semi-structured interviews, subsequent to which data were qualitatively analysed. participants this study made use of two groups of participants: slps and clients/ caregivers. clients with voice disorders (including laryngectomies), dysphagia or cleft lip and/or palate were the focus of the study. this is because clients’ understanding and recall of information relating to these disorders is most critical to compliance with treatment (fagan, lentin, oyarzabal, isaacs & sellars, 2002; low, wyles, wilkinson & sainsbury, 2001; sataloff, 2006). selection criteria for slps were that they had to be practising currently in the greater cape town area, and had to have at least 1 year’s post-qualification experience in managing at least one of the disorders that formed the focus of the study. selection criteria for clients were that they had to have one of the selected disorders or be a caregiver of a child with an unrepaired cleft lip and/or palate or a voice or swallowing disorder. they had to speak a language other than english as their first language because issues which might arise from the language divide between professionals and clients increase the potential for misunderstandings. clients also had to have a poor socio-economic background (i.e. live in informal settlements or rural areas), as these clients might be more likely to experience difficulties with compliance as a result of poverty (kagee, 2004). in addition, clients had to have low health and general literacy skills as determined through an informal health literacy screening tool (see appendix a). it was important to know the level of health/general literacy skills of participants, as clients with low health/general literacy skills typically have fewer means to improve their health literacy skills or review health advice because they cannot understand or read health care materials, thus indicating their need for additional strategies to assist them in this regard. clients were excluded when they had any additional cognitive, psychological or intellectual disorders, as these were likely to affect their ability to give consent and their ability to express their opinion and perceptions. one reason for including slps as well as clients in this study was because little research has focused on investigating slps as well as clients as key role-players in the clinical process (hester & stevens-ratchford, 2009). few studies mentioned caregivers and the issues that come with low health literacy and problems in recalling clinical information. for this reason the present study also included the caregivers of children. clients and slps were recruited at two large public hospitals and a community rehabilitation centre in the western cape. sampling purposive sampling was used with participants selected based on the researchers’ knowledge of their characteristics and the purpose of the study. a total of four participants were needed (i.e. one participant with a voice disorder, one with dysphagia, one with a cleft lip and/or palate and one with a laryngectomy). a small sample size was sufficient as this is an exploratory, qualitative study where the focus was on richness and detail of discussions. procedure this study was conducted in accordance with the declaration of helsinki. the principles that were followed were the principle of respect for people (which includes confidentiality and their autonomy), the principle of non-maleficence/beneficence and the principle of justice. each participant had to be legally competent, informed and show comprehension before they were invited to sign a consent form voluntarily. where people could not write, they were able to sign with a thumbprint. the consent forms were read out and explained to them in their preferred language. a trained interpreter was used for this to ensure that the content of the consent form was translated accurately. a combination of strategies was devised to assist in: (i) maximising health literacy and client recall; and (ii) ensuring an open channel of contact between clients and their slps post consultation. advantages and disadvantages of various strategies (see table i), information drawn from the literature as well as suggestions by kessels (2003) and rao (2007) were considered when deciding on strategies to be trialled. the following strategies were selected to be trialled in combination: (i) audio-recordings in the form of a cd/cassette, (ii) ‘please call me’ sms/ text message, and (iii) telephonic follow-up. the reason for this is that an audio-recording of the consultation in the form of a cd/cassette is only efficient in assisting health literacy and recall of clinical information, whereas ‘please call me’ sms/text message and consequential telephonic follow-up would only serve to allow for routine reminders, enquiries, discussions and feedback post consultation. in south africa, radio ownership increased from 73% in 2001 to 76.6% in 2007 (south africa online, 2010) and these days even cheap radios include a cd player. even people without their own cd player are likely to be able to use a neighbour’s or relative’s cd-player. the ‘please call me’ sms function is a standard message (no typing required) that can be sent free of charge to another person’s mobile phone in order to alert them of this individual’s request for telephonic contact. low-tech (e.g. written, illustrative) strategies were not included in this study as these were already used by the slps. however where slps provided low-tech strategies, the proposed high-tech strategies were always added as a further strategy. even though only the hightech strategies were focused on in the study, this mix of low-tech and high-tech strategies ensured the provision of visual, auditory and tactile elements in information exchange during and post consultation. trialling the use of technology/strategies to maximise health literacy and client recall of clinical information was a key concept to be introduced at a client’s first consultation, given that this is typically where health literacy, recall and other variables to compliance determine clients’ understanding, eventual recall and resultant adherence to treatment regimens. firstly, slps were familiarised with the procedures of the three chosen strategies to prevent potential problems from occurring during consultations. slps conducted initial assessments according to their routine clinical procedure and made recommendations to their client. whenever important information and recommendations were to be provided, the slp gave a signal to the researcher to audio-record that information. when all information was recorded and the consultation was concluded, this audio-recording was given to the client in addition to any low-tech materials that the slp usually provides. the purpose and use of this recording together with the use of the ‘please call me’ function of their mobile phone was explained by the researcher. clients were also given a date for a follow-up appointment (after at least 3 but not more than 6 weeks). the reason for keeping the time interval below 6 weeks was that it has been reported that recall decreases over time even when strategies have been used to improve recall of clinical information and health literacy. this time period gave the client an opportunity to independently manage their own health while using the proposed strategies. the researcher made notes based on information 26 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 health literacy and client recall from the assessment and recommendations so as to have a record of each participant’s treatment regimen for comparisons with the slp’s observations and for later use in data analysis. data collection the outcomes of the technology/strategies employed during the period between the first consultation and the first follow-up appointment were evaluated from both the slp’s and client’s perspective. firstly, clients were interviewed through short semistructured interviews to probe more deeply into their experiences and views on the effectiveness of the strategies in assisting with understanding and recalling clinical information. subsequently, a questionnaire using a likert scale and open-ended questions as well as a subsequent semi-structured interview was conducted with slps. the purpose of the questionnaire was to assist in organising data and further in-depth discussions while allowing slps time to think about their responses. the subsequent interview gave the researcher an opportunity to probe more deeply into the positive or negative experiences reported by the slp after having used the strategies. analysis an immersion/crystallisation style was used when analysing the data. for this purpose, data of slp and client individual interviews were transcribed. patterns that emerged from the data had to be identified and matched with patterns in the literature review/other scientific knowledge so as to enhance internal validity. themes suggested by ammenwerth, brender, nykänen, prokosch, rigby and talmon (2004) were considered, adapted and combined to form five themes against which the data were analysed. the themes were: 1. improvement of knowledge and recall of medical information (long and short term). 2. satisfaction related to the value that is placed on the strategies or the extent to which the participants found them to be helpful. 3. is the combination of the strategies usable in the intended environment and for the intended purpose? if not, what are slps’ and clients’ attitudes towards using the strategies in future and suggestions on improvements? 4. do users need more training and guidance in order to use the strategies appropriately? 5. possible effects that the combination of these strategies has on structural or process quality (e.g. time saving, clinical workflow). results case study 1 (client with a laryngectomy) in this case study, strategies that were used included counselling, illustrations, audiorecording, the option of a ‘please call me’ sms/text message and telephonic followup. the client reported that she forgot some details when she eventually arrived at home, which is when she used the audio-recording to refresh the information in her mind, thus indicating her need for a strategy to review information once she was at home. she also found it helpful to listen to a voice during the day when she was alone at home, to review information and for reassurance. she explained: p1: during the weeks when i was so alone, i would sit and i put the tape on and just listen. r: so was it also just that you could listen to the speech therapist’s voice? p1: yes. nevertheless, this particular client continued to have poor recall and health literacy skills, possibly related to emotional aspects, poor social factors and additional stressors of daily life. she spent much of her energy on longing for her previous life and coming to terms with her present situation, rather than performing self-care and managing her own health and future through the use of strategies. she felt hopeless and confused, which may have further clouded her judgement and prevented her from effectively managing her own health. the client also struggled to deal with her partner’s rejection and reluctance to accept her condition. this agrees with a finding by bunning (2004), who explained that knowing what a child or partner was like before being affected by a disability can make it difficult for significant others to accept the ‘new’ person, which in turn makes it more difficult for the client to be enthusiastic about complying with treatment regimens. it is not known whether or not the client would have been better equipped to manage her own health after she had time to deal with her situation, regain confidence and t ab le i i. c as e st ud y 1 c as e nu m be r sp ee ch -l an gu ag e pa th ol og y li te ra cy s ki lls r es id es in h om e la ng ua ge em pl oy m en t li vi ng s it ua ti on r ep or te d he al th li te ra cy se rv ic e pr ov id ed fo r sk il ls c as e 1 po st op er at iv e co un se lli ng a fte r to ta l c an no t r ea d or w ri te r ur al a re a a fr ik aa ns u ne m pl oy ed st ay ed w ith h er h us ba nd sa id th at s he h ad g re at d iffi cu lty (f em al e ag ed 5 6) la ry ng ec to m y in a ny la ng ua ge in in fo rm al h ou si ng w ith n o un de rs ta nd in g te rm s an d m os t ru nn in g w at er o n a fa rm o ut in fo rm at io n ab ou t he r co nd iti on of th e c ap e to w n ar ea tr ea ti ng s lp ye ar s of e xp er ie nc e in th e fi el d an d in st ra te gi es th at a re u su al ly e m pl oy ed h om e la ng ua ge em pl oy ed a t w or ki ng in p ub lic h ea lt h fe m al e 27 • v er ba l i ns tr uc tio n en gl is h te rt ia ry h os pi ta l • e du ca tio n • d em on st ra tio n • r ep et iti on • i llu st ra tio ns • a ss is t w hi le in de pe nd en ce is e st ab lis he d • i nf or m at io n br oc hu re s an d pa m ph le ts • p ro vi de s w or k nu m be r an d oc ca si on al ly m ob ile p ho ne n um be r vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 27 health literacy and client recall practise using the combined strategies to her advantage. the researchers and the slp wondered whether or not the effectiveness of the combined strategies might be realised at a later stage. discussions with the slp continued to investigate what could be done differently for clients with laryngectomies who are illiterate, come from rural areas and have a disadvantaged socio-economic background. the slp saw the value of the combined strategies, but she was aware that they might not be a single universal technique to use with any client. she explained: with laryngectomies i don’t really know. i think if they don’t have easy access because they often need medical attention … you know none of the strategies might be superb … because it’s not just depending on them. it’s not like a dysphagia where you say; remember to put your head down when you swallow! ahm … there can be infections, there can be small stomas, there can be … prostheses fall out and then what you’ve taught them is just for if everything is fine. if everything isn’t fine they need medical attention so … therefore, no single strategy might be effective because there will always be unforeseen problems that need immediate attention from various professionals. this highlights the need for regular contact – face to face or through sms/text message or telephone. finally, it was observed that the slp was caring and spoke to the client in a manner which was clear and in accordance with the client’s level of language, a factor which was viewed to play a significant role in this case. case study 2 (caregivers of a child with cleft palate) in this case study, strategies that were used were counselling, an audiorecording and the option of a ‘please call me’ sms/text message and telephonic follow-up. management in this case was effective; determined by the slp’s and the client’s accounts, as well as their ability to follow treatment guidelines. however, it was not clear whether the success was due to: (i) the combined strategies being of real assistance; (ii) the caregivers being insightful and motivated from the beginning; or (iii) a combination of both. nevertheless, the clients did not have any written information to refer to, which suggests that the combined strategies were important when clients wanted to review information. in addition to being able to review information, the caregivers felt it was also beneficial to share information with others. the mother reported that her neighbour had commented on the use of the audio-recording as follows: she say it’s nice, doctor give to you the cd, reminding you every time your baby … they say yes it’s nice talk (not audible). this case provided a contrast to case study 1. in the first case, the client experienced socio-emotional difficulties that impacted on her compliance, but in this case the parents were practical and pro-active about their child’s future health. the slp saw the value of the combined strategies but had concerns about their future use. among the reasons for this concern was the issue of interpreters and that she felt the strategies were not practical because they would take time and she would have to invest in a system that would record and burn cds. she would also always have to find a quiet room, which is not easy in a hospital, and it would require a lot of additional planning which would be more of a burden than of assistance. she further explained that these particular clients might also struggle with making sense of the information later at home, when it is out of context. alternatively, the slp suggested that only a mobile phone number instead of a cd be provided as it was quicker and easier, there was less room for misunderstandings and no need for additional expensive equipment to be bought. however, if clients do not contact the slp to ask for clarifications, they might misunderstand or not remember information and implement the wrong feeding technique. in the questionnaire, this slp indicated that there is a difference between disorders and the need for the combined strategies, which is similar to what the slp in case study 1 suggested. this slp explained that in cases of voice clients for example, cds with clinical information would be more practical where principles of therapy are more generic and you can easily record and make sense of them. another example that she provided was that of articulation therapy for children, where she could record a session and the caregiver could follow the whole programme at home. in cases of clients with dysphagia or laryngectomies, this is more difficult as disorders are more acute or critical, and hands-on guidance, as well as practical demonstration, is needed. in these cases, visual tools like a video-recording would probably be more appropriate because information is presented visually and in context, whereas an audiorecording might be less clear especially if out of context. however, she explained that having to create video-recordings might be just as impractical as audio-recordings. it would initially take additional consultation time and she would have to invest in a system that would record videos or dvds. this question of whether this specific combination of strategies would perhaps be more effective with disorders that are less severe, complex and acute stood out after having analysed these two case studies. case study 3 (adult client with dysphagia) in this case study, strategies that were used were counselling, illustrations, written information in point-form, an audio-recording, the option of a ‘please call me’ sms/text message and telephonic follow-up. one advantage of the combined strategies which stood out most in this case study was that the strategies and materials provided clients with an opportunity to comprehend and recall aims and rationales for treatments. the slp in this case explained that: … the client seemed like he had more resources to work with, he was more motivated to do his task … he was more motivated because table iii. case study 2 case number speech-language literacy resides home employment living reported health pathology service skills in language situation literacy skills provided for case 2 provision of information, no english literacy urban area somali unemployed lived in a small house able to ask important (caregiver aged 33) counselling and feeding skills, but proficient with the husband’s questions during recommendations for in somali brother and his family consultations with premature infant with only an occasional cleft palate misunderstanding treating slp years of experience in strategies that are home employed at the field and in working usually employed language in public health female 14 • verbal feedback • counselling • information brochures and pamphlets • provides work and occasionally mobile phone number english tertiary hospital 28 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 health literacy and client recall he could take ownership and responsibility for his therapy because he had something in his hand you know to look back at and to refer back to all the time. so for me i feel that now, where you actually give the client a tape or you give the client the page, then it’s them taking the responsibility and saying; ok i need to work on my thing now. so i think it’s quite beneficial because if the clients leave without something, then i often find that they forget everything and they don’t take that responsibility. when asked whether they would have still forgotten even in the presence of having the illustrations and the recommendations in pointform, the clients explained: the tape provides more clarity. look we got the exercises but this is just a picture and the picture cannot speak … but the patient and the speech therapist speak with each other. she explains what is going on and he follows her advice. there is an understanding between the two because what is discussed in the session is recorded on here. it is something that happens in real-time. furthermore, the couple felt that access to ‘please call me’ sms/text message and regular telephonic contact would have been most beneficial initially when the client had first come out of the hospital. he had experienced various difficulties with his speech in addition to frequent aspiration and it would have made them feel safer knowing that they could receive immediate support when needed. here, it is indicated that at the initial stages of treatment, clients’ need for professional support and empowering strategies to manage their health is vital. the slp thought the combined strategies were appropriate for all disorders listed; namely dysphagia, voice disorders, laryngectomy and cleft lip and/or palate management. all these disorders require the therapist to obtain regular feedback from clients because the status of these disorders frequently changes and new difficulties arise. for example, there might have been spontaneous recovery or regression in the client’s therapeutic process. she explained: especially with the dysphagia clients, i noticed that they often recover quickly or they have different problems … so i feel they need to be monitored closely because maybe on the first day you see that your aims are specific and then the next time that you see them the aims are different because … there has been spontaneous recovery, there has been some regression. so, in order for my therapy process to be guided, i need to get that constant feedback from the client all the time. when aims become obsolete in the event of regression or improvement of a disorder, clients can review the information, realise that it is not sufficient any more, and become conscious of their need to contact their slp to solve current problems. she agreed that this is also where ‘please call me’ sms/text message and subsequent telephonic follow-up become central. she was worried that when this does not occur, clients become overwhelmed by the changes and resultant needs associated with their disorders if not followed-up immediately. … i feel worried that their disorder might change and they get lost in the system and then who does the follow-up, if we aren’t there to do the follow-up for them? so it becomes a critical issue. this adds to findings by zagaria (2006) who found that, when lacking appropriate support, clients become overwhelmed when having to manage their own health and by the pressures of achieving therapy goals. the above perspective provides a reason why these strategies would be effective in the treatment of the disorders focused upon in this study, as well as in any other disorders treated in speech-language pathology. overall, the slp was enthusiastic when asked whether or not she felt that she would like to routinely use the combined strategies. she explained: … i would definitely use the strategy if i had something set up here at the centre and say for instance they gave me this in a budget and they gave me the resources to be able to do it, then i would have done it. table iv. case study 3 case number speech-language literacy resides home employment living reported health pathology service skills in language situation literacy skills provided for case 3 was educated about finished school informal afrikaans unemployed his wife, a domestic they explained that (male aged 41) reasons for swallowing at grade 4. settlement worker, was the sole they occasionally difficulties and received says he has provider of the family had difficulties recommendations for difficulty reading (with two children). understanding feeding as well as exercises complex text they lived in informal some written or for dysarthria housing with no running verbal information water or electricity. during the day, the client’s sister took care of him. his wife sometimes pushed the client in his wheelchair more than 5 km to the clinic. when they had enough money, they would hire a taxi. the only mobile phone which they owned had been lost recently. the sister’s mobile phone was therefore used in cases of emergency treating slp years of experience in the strategies that are home employed at field and in working in usually employed language public health female 2 • illustrations english & community • keeping aims at a minimum afrikaans clinic • repetitions • using teach-back method • written instructions for caregivers vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 29 health literacy and client recall even though the slp saw the value of the combined strategies, she had some concerns which would have to be addressed. for example, she emphasised that she would have to set up a system for recording information that was not intrusive to the client, and instead of having the ‘please call me’ smss come to her personal mobile phone, have a computer system at the centre that would receive smss/text messages. she explained that this is important as she works in a team setup and clients frequently have difficulties that have to be addressed by a team of experts. this further emphasised the usefulness of the ‘please call me’ sms/text message and telephonic follow-up when clients are managed by various health professionals. the slp in this case study was noticeably more positive, motivated and enthusiastic about implementing the combined strategies in future compared with the therapists in the first and second case study. case study 4 (adult client with a voice disorder) in this case study, strategies that were used were counselling, written information in point form, an audio-recording and the option of a ‘please call me’ sms/text message and telephonic follow-up. the client reported that even though she had understood everything once she had left the consultation for the first time, she felt the need to listen to the audio-recorded information frequently because it contained further details than the written information provided for her. she explained: you know i got all this information on a piece of paper but it is better for me to put on the tape and listen to it ... the additional detail to information that is on the tape is helpful yes. the client also found that the combined strategies gave her the opportunity to manage her voice disorder on a long-term rather than a once-off basis. as a result of dedicating time, sitting down and following written instructions while listening to the cassette, she may have felt more as though she was doing a whole treatment session rather than just reading a sheet of written clinical information in point-form which could be less engaging after a period of time, or it might not be appropriate to her learning style (e.g. she might prefer a range of audio, visual or pictorial materials). one slp opinion that stood out from this discussion was that, irrespective of the presenting disorders, clients (at the institution where she works) all have low socio-economic backgrounds and similar education levels, which is why the combined strategies would serve them all in maximising health literacy skills, recall and compliance – and not just clients with selected disorders. the slp explained: … they will use their last money because they are concerned about their health … so you can’t come and waste their time. you need to give as much as you can especially for the people who live far … they need to leave feeling that it was worth that r60 or whatever that they gave out. health professionals have to make every effort to ensure that clients understand their treatment regimen and can manage their own health when they return home so that motivation and treatment compliance can be improved. this has also been mentioned by most therapists in this study which confirms slps’ awareness of the importance of these variables. the slp raised some concerns that would have to be addressed if the combined strategies were to be used in future: for example, (i) that clients might not have the insight to realise that they need to question certain aspects of therapy or acknowledge their need to contact the therapist for assistance; and (ii) when clients have a problem, they usually consult someone in their community who they regard as being wise (e.g. a traditional healer or a community leader), which could ultimately result in the combined strategies being ineffectual as they would not be used. the researcher and the slp agreed that, should the combined strategies be implemented at an institution, it would allow people to become used to their presence and they might use a combination of strategies or just some part of them more readily and effectively. she also highlighted that it is important to understand that cultural influences are particularly strong in rural areas. in this case, the slp would have to investigate a client’s needs and preferences after which a joint decision could be made on a strategy which allows for regular contact with the slp and also maximises that client’s health literacy, recall and resultant compliance to treatment. once more, this slp was noticeably more positive, motivated and enthusiastic about implementing the combined strategies in future compared with the therapists in the first and second case studies. discussion management of three out of the four case studies was judged to be effective as determined by the slps’ and the clients’ accounts, as well as their ability to follow treatment guidelines. various factors played a role in each particular case, but subjective opinions largely showed that the strategies assisted clients in maximising health literacy and client recall, while ensuring an open channel of contact between clients and their slps post consultation. table v. case study 4 case number speech-language literacy resides home employment living reported health pathology service skills in language situation literacy skills provided for case 4 counselling for the moderate literacy urban area afrikaans employed as she was in possession she explained that (female aged 58) prevention of vocal skills in english in a house a tea-lady of a cd/tape-player she occasionally abuse and misuse and afrikaans as well as a house had difficulties in telephone and would learning about her occasionally use her or other family daughter’s mobile members’ medical phone if needed conditions because of difficulties in understanding medical written or verbal information treating slp years of experience strategies that she usually uses home employed at in the field and in language working in public health female 8 • written instructions english tertiary hospital • teach-back method • informing caregivers and significant others about the client’s disorder and treatment 30 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 health literacy and client recall clients in all four case studies listened to the audio-recorded information and were aided through being able to review the detailed conversation. furthermore, clients in case studies 1 and 2 found listening to a voice more reassuring, personal and human compared with written information. these are important findings, as much of the literature usually provided to clients goes unread (kessels, 2003). as described by osborne (2008a), some participants were reluctant to get into telephonic contact. in some cases there were technical issues, whereas in other cases participants just did not feel the need to contact their slp even though their follow-up appointment showed that it would have been helpful had they contacted their slp for guidance on selected issues. in most cases, provision of the combined strategies was time well spent, especially when clients were at ‘high risk’ and had ready access to the equipment needed to utilise the strategies. some slps indicated the cost-effectiveness of using the combined strategies in that if clients were to start using these strategies early in their treatment process, recovery could be achieved more quickly as clients can review information in their own time and through regular telephonic contact, clients can be assisted continually, not just at follow-up appointments. in addition, the strategies would not require expensive equipment. for example, most professionals have access to the internet and generally clients have access to phones, mobile phones and cd players (kreutzer, 2009). cassettes, which were used in some of the case studies, are rapidly becoming outdated, which is why cds/dvds, widely and relatively inexpensively available in south africa, would be more appropriate. an initial investment would need to be made for a portable cd burner and audio-recorder. however, this equipment should be useful for many other tasks in hospitals/clinics. slps would need time to develop the best equipment and procedure (through clinical trial and error) so that they could make informed and effective decisions for when and how to use these strategies without them becoming an added burden. all the effort would be a waste of time if interpreters are not readily available, both at times of audio-recordings and during telephonic follow-up. furthermore, slps made it clear that the combined strategies are not a blanket technique for all clients. clients would need to (i) have access and experience in using the equipment needed to utilise the combined strategies; and (ii) be insightful, motivated and have enough problem-solving skills to use the strategies effectively. a client’s stage of recovery and resulting coping skills, as well as cultural influences and familial circumstances, are also variables that have to be considered as possibly interfering with effective use of strategies by clients. when considering all participants’ opinions, it became clear that the clients’ overall experience and value of the combined strategies, was positive and that without them they would have had difficulties in dealing with the information and managing their health post consultation. however, at times it seemed that clients would be satisfied with any strategy which is novel and makes them feel confident in managing their own (or their child’s) health. slps were generally positive about the combined strategies. slps of case studies 3 and 4 were noticeably more positive, motivated and enthusiastic about implementing the combined strategies in future than the therapists in the first and second case studies. it was not clear whether this was because they were younger and had spent less time in practice than the former slps, who were more wary of the challenges faced and the role of the combined strategies. therapists who have been in the field for many years have settled perceptions about the most appropriate information exchange in practice. they have had much experience of clients’ lack of adherence to treatment regimens, despite their efforts to assist clients in complying. on the other hand, younger therapists may like to experiment with various avenues of information exchange with their clients, which could be a reason for their enthusiasm about the combined strategies. clinical implications slps need to be aware that motivation and insight may impact considerably on compliance and treatment outcomes – with or without strategies – and therefore should also consider ways of maximising these. slps would have to investigate obstacles to compliance, a clients’ stage of recovery (and resultant coping skills), personal situations, learning styles and ways of thinking and problem-solving when wanting to maximise information uptake. subsequently, both parties could jointly decide on a strategy that allows for regular contact with the slp and also maximises that client’s health literacy and recall post consultation. it would be inappropriate to expect that the combined strategies could solely assure that health literacy and recall are maximised. a caring and supportive therapist is necessary for clients to feel motivated and confident to take control of the management of their health. at the same time clients have to be insightful enough to realise when they need assistance and make contact so that no time is wasted and their condition does not deteriorate. therefore, when slps identify a client who is at ‘high risk’ for non-compliance, they should initially contact the client to encourage recall and problem solving and at the same time teach the client to initiate contact. slps need to acknowledge that any strategy which makes clients feel confident because they are equipped and thus empowered to manage their (or their child’s) health is vital when wanting to assure continued service delivery and success in treatment (moon, cheng, patel, baumhaft & scheidt, 1998). these combined strategies might not only have different functions and possibly different outcomes for different disorders, but they might also have different outcomes at different stages of a client’s recovery. the strategies should be adapted or provided as they have been proposed, to most clients who seek speech-language pathology treatment for any disorder, especially those with low literacy skills and who do not have the financial means to attend frequent therapy sessions or contact their slp telephonically. a preliminary framework (see appendix b) could serve as a decisionmaking framework for slps who want to maximise health literacy, recall and compliance in clients. this framework is preliminary because it only focuses on the disorders and settings from this preliminary study. future research exploring the combined strategies for other disorders in different settings would assist in making this framework more holistic and complete. future research should also investigate a larger more representative sample including a range of disorders in various settings across the country and at different stages of a client’s recovery. clients from high socio-economic backgrounds with well-developed literacy skills may be affected by some of the variables to compliance that have been identified in this study and in the literature. participants with good health literacy skills should be included in future studies to identify the needs and issues that play a role in their compliance. further research topics could include (i) whether or not the issues that these clients experience are similar or different to those of clients with low health literacy skills; and (ii) which strategies could be of assistance to their compliance with speech-language pathology treatments. limitations this study was limited in that the long-term effectiveness of the combined strategies was not explored. ideally these strategies should have been used with clients right through their treatment process. another procedure that could have been conducted was to perform a quiz with clients right after the consultation and then again before the next follow-up appointment to generate objective data that would show (i) how much information the client initially understood and recalled; and (ii) how much information they could recall and describe after having used the strategies at home. vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 31 health literacy and client recall conclusion this study provided insight into various strategies that are available for information exchange. almost all the interventions that aim to improve compliance are complex (kagee, 2004). there is no one solution or universal tool for maximising health literacy and recall in the presence of various factors that impact the utilisation of the combined strategies and compliance. it depends on each client and their treating slp which aspect of the combined strategies they see fit to use. future challenges for slps will be to effectively manage barriers such as clients’ varying levels of literacy, motivation, insight and problemsolving skills; the lack of support from families; the need for trained interpreters; and lack of funds and resources. it is important that slps not only consider health literacy and client recall issues in their everyday practice but that this profession with its methods of analysis and sensitivity to the communication process can unquestionably make a contribution to health literacy and client recall in general, particularly in south africa with its disease burden and where there are many barriers to care. the cycle of low health literacy and education levels, poor income and health and the inaccessibility to information technology can only be broken if information is brought to those who are underserved. acknowledgements acknowledgements to all the participants who volunteered their time, and especially to the therapists who assisted in finding clients. thanks also to martha geiger and vivienne norman for comments on an earlier draft. references ammenwerth, e., brender, j., nykänen, p., prokosch, h. u., rigby, m., & talmon, j. (2004). visions and strategies to improve evaluation of health information systems. reflections and lessons based on the his-eval workshop in innsbruck [electronic version]. international journal of medical informatics, 73, 479-491. bellardie, h. h., & harris, a. m. p. (2008). what do parents of newborn babies with cleft lip and/or palate want to know? does the leaflet from the cleft lip and palate unit at red cross children’s hospital fulfil these needs, and what is its grading for ‘readability’? south african journal of child health, 2(3), 125-128. big media publishers. the languages of south africa. retrieved on 5 february 2008 from http://www.southafrica.info/ess_info/sa_glance/demographics/language.htm bunning, k. (2004). speech and language therapy intervention: frameworks and processes. london: whurr publishers. chang, m., & kelly, a. e. (2007). patient education: addressing cultural diversity and health literacy issues. urologic nursing, 27(5), 411417. chew, l. d., bradley, k. a., & boyko, e. j. (2004). brief questions to identify patients with inadequate health literacy [electronic version]. special articles: health literacy and family medicine, 36(8), 588-594. cilliers, b. (2005). talking about medical talk: exploring experiences regarding communication in hiv/aids health care in a tertiary health care setting. unpublished master’s thesis, university of cape town. cooper-patrick, l., gallo, j. j., gonzales, j. j., thi vu, h., powe, n. r., nelson, c., & ford, d. e. (1999). race, gender, and partnership in the patient-physician relationship [electronic version]. journal of the american medical association, 282(6), 583-589. fagan, j. j., lentin, r., oyarzabal, m. f., isaacs, s., & sellars, s. l. (2002). tracheoesophageal speech in a developing world community [electronic version]. archives of otolaryngology – head & neck surgery, 128, 50-53. gal, i., & prigat, a. (2005). why organisations continue to create patient information leaflets with readability and usability problems: an exploratory study. health education research, 20(4), 485-493. harris, j. l., fleming, v. b., & mcdougall, g. j. (2003, november). effects of text and reader variables on understanding health information. poster presentation at the annual convention of the american speech-language-hearing association, chicago. hester, e. j., & stevens-ratchford, r. (2009). health literacy and the role of the speechlanguage pathologist. american journal of speech-language pathology, 18, 180-191. institute of medicine. (2004). health literacy: a prescription to end confusion. retrieved on 2 february 2008 from http://www.iom.edu/object.file/master/19/736/health%20 literacy%20final.pdf interagency coordinating council. (2002). adult basic and literacy education. (a report on health literacy.) retrieved on 4 april 2008 from http://www.able.state.pa.us/able/lib/able/ healthlit_2002.pdf international institute for communication and development (iicd). (2006). sms technology for tb treatment. retrieved on 28 march 2008 from http://www.comminit. com/en/node/147683 internet world stats. (2010). south africa – mobile market, overview and statistics report. retrieved 22 april 2008, from http://www.internetworldstats.com/af/za.htm kagee, a. (2004). treatment adherence in south african primary healthcare [electronic version]. south african family practice, 46(10), 26-30. kahn, a., & pannbacker, m. (2000). readability of educational materials for patients with cleft lip and palate and their families [electronic version]. american journal of speechlanguage pathology, 9, 3-9. kelly, p. a., & haidet, p. (2007). physician overestimation of patient literacy: a potential source of healthcare disparities. patient education and counseling, 66, 119-122. kessels, r. p. c. (2003). patients’ memory for medical information [electronic version]. journal of the royal society of medicine, 96(5), 219-222. kreutzer, t. (2009). generation mobile: online and digital media usage on mobile phones among low-income urban youth in south africa. unpublished master’s thesis, university of cape town. low, j., wyles, c., wilkinson, t., & sainsbury, r. (2001). the effect of compliance on clinical outcomes of patients with dysphagia on videofluoroscopy [electronic version]. dysphagia, 16, 123-127. marfo, k. (1993). families in non-western cultures: africa as a case example. in j. l. paul & r. j. simeonsson (eds.), children with special needs: family, culture and society (2nd ed., pp. 147-163). fort worth: harcourt-brace college publishers. maxwell, d. l., & satake, e. (2006). research and statistical methods in communication sciences and disorders. canada: thomson delmar learning. mcdonald, h. p., garg, a. x., & haynes, r. b. (2002). interventions to enhance patient adherence to medication prescriptions: a scientific review [electronic version]. journal of the american medical association, 288(22), 2868-2879. moon, r. y., cheng, t. l., patel, k. m., baumhaft, k., & scheidt, p. c. (1998). parental literacy level and understanding of medical information [electronic version]. pediatrics, 102(2), 25-30. nelson mandela foundation. (2004). how far have we come? reflections from the first 10 years of basic education transformation. retrieved 2 february 2008 from http://www. polity.org.za/attachment.php?aa_id=2848 north, n., cornbleet, m. a., knowles, g., & leonard, r. c. (1992). information giving in oncology: a preliminary study of tape-recorder use [electronic version]. british journal of clinical psychology, 31, 357-359. osborne, h. (2008a). making the phone a useful tool for working with patients. oncall magazine: an online magazine for healthcare professionals. retrieved 2 august 2008 from http://www.healthliteracy.com/article.asp?pageid=7849 osborne, h. (2008b). in other words ... how memory affects health understanding. oncall magazine: an online magazine for healthcare professionals. retrieved 2 august 2008 from http://www.healthliteracy.com/article.asp?pageid=7849 parker, r. m., ratzan, s. c., & lurie, n. (2003). health literacy: a policy challenge for advancing high-quality healthcare [electronic version]. health affairs, 22(4), 147. parkin, t., & skinner, t. c. (2003). discrepancies between patient and professionals recall and perception of an outpatient consultation [electronic version]. diabetic medicine, 20, 909-914. penn, c., frankel, t., watermeyer, j., & muller, m. (2009). informed consent and aphasia: evidence of pitfalls in the process. aphasiology, 23(1), 3-32. penn, c. (2007). factors affecting the success of mediated medical interviews in south africa [electronic version]. current allergy & clinical immunology, 20(2), 66-72. pretorius, l. (2004). an introduction to adult literacy in the african context [electronic version]. language matters: studies in the languages of southern africa: adult literacy in the african context: special issue, 35(2), 343-347. rao, p. r. (2007, may). health literacy: the cornerstone of patient safety [electronic version]. the asha leader, 8-21. ross, j. (2007). health literacy and its influence on patient safety [electronic version]. journal of perianesthesia nursing, 22(3), 220-222. sanders, l., & brosco, j. (2005). low caregiver health literacy: a risk factor for child access to a medical home [electronic version]. pediatric academic society, 57, 2772. santo, a., laizner, a. m., & shohet, l. (2005). exploring the value of audiotapes for health literacy: a systemic review [electronic version]. patient education and counseling, 58, 235-243. sataloff, r. t. (2006). voice impairment, disability, handicap and medical/legal evaluation. in j. s. rubin, r. t. sataloff & g. s. korovin (eds.), diagnosis and treatment of voice disorders (3rd ed., pp. 319-327). san diego: plural publishing. schwartz, t. (2004). communication in health within the south african context: current practices employed across three levels of health care. unpublished master’s thesis, university of cape town. south africa online. (2010). communications. retrieved 10 may 2010 from http://www. southafrica.co.za/about-south-africa/science-and-technology/communications/ unesco institute for lifelong learning. (2010). adult literacy and skills training programme (alstp) country profile: south africa. retrieved 8 june 2010 from http:// www.unesco.org/uil/litbase/?menu=4&programme=52 williams, m. v., baker, d. w., parker, r. m., & nurss, j. r. (1998). relationship of functional health literacy to patients’ knowledge of their chronic disease [electronic version]. archives of internal medicine, 158, 166-172. yin, h. s., forbis, s. g., & dreyer, b. p. (2007, august). health literacy and pediatric health [electronic version]. current problems in pediatric adolescent healthcare, 37, 258-286. zagaria, m.e. (2006). low health literacy: a safety concern among the elderly [electronic version]. u.s. pharmacist, 31,28-34. retrieved 8 june 2010 from http://www. uspharmacist.com/content/d/senior%20care/c/11540/ zhang, c., & bennet, t. (2001). multicultural views of disability: implications for early intervention professionals [electronic version]. infant-toddler intervention: the transdisciplinary journal, 11, 143-154. 32 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 health literacy and client recall appendix a. informal health literacy screening tool adapted from chew et al. (2004) and yin et al. (2007) 1. how often are medical forms and pamphlets written in a way that is difficult for you to read and understand? always often sometimes occasionally never 2. how often are medication labels written in a way that is difficult for you to read and understand? always often sometimes occasionally never 3. how often are medical forms difficult for you to understand and fill out? always often sometimes occasionally never 4. how often are written recommendations for therapy/treatment difficult for you to understand? always often sometimes occasionally never 5. how often are verbal recommendations for therapy/treatment difficult for you to understand? always often sometimes occasionally never 6. how often do you have problems getting to your clinic appointments at the right time because of difficulty understanding written instructions about when and why you must come to the clinic? always often sometimes occasionally never 7. how often do you have problems learning about your (or your child’s) medical condition because of difficulty understanding written or verbal information? always often sometimes occasionally never 8. how often do you need someone (like a family member, friend, hospital/clinic worker or caregiver) to help you read and understand hospital materials? always often sometimes occasionally never appendix b. preliminary framework for speech-language pathologists when making practical decisions to assist client health literacy and recall o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y negative practice in dysphemic therapy a case history by f. t. lubinsky, b.a., log. (rand). , peter smith was 13 years old when he came to the speech clinic. he was in standard v. he lived in a small country town where his father practised as a doctor. he displayed severe clonic blocks in his speech, and had very marked secondary symptoms, e.g., before every block he gasped for breath and said 'uh-uh" on a rising and falling tone. during the stuttering block his right leg moved jerkily, and his right hand tapped his side vigorously. mrs. smith brought peter into town at the beginning of july, 1954, to attend the speech clinic. she said that she and her husband were very concerned about peter's speech, which had become much worse since he was in standard v. his teacher at that time was very strict, and peter was afraid of him. in the initial interview mrs. smith said : "my two daughters who are younger than peter are cleverer than he is at school, and this gives him an inferiority complex." the interviewer noted that peter was extremely shy and appeared to lack self-confidence. he was very anxious about his speech, and very eager to improve. the following recommendations were made after the initial interview : (1) peter should have intensive speech therapy during the holidays whenever he could come to johannesburg. (2) the danger of their anxiety relaying itself to peter, and thus making his speech "worse, was explained to the parents. they were advised to ignore his speech symptom.' tests given at the commencement of therapy. (1) clarke thurstone neurotic tendency test. results showed him to be more neurotic than the average. (2) iowa attitude scale, where his score indicated that he had a very bad attitude to his speech. 2, 3). (3) iowa rating of severity of stuttering. he was rated as "7. very severe: stuttering on more than 25 per cent, of the words very conspicuous tension: blocks average more than 4 seconds: very conspicuous distracting sounds and facial grimaces : very conspicuous distracting associated movements of body, arms, or legs." (4) recordings were taken of his speech in reading and conversation, in which he averaged twenty blocks a minute. he used so many secondary symptoms that his speech was almost unintelligible. therapy altogether he had four periods of intensive therapy, which co-incided with his school holidays. (1) 26 hours ; (2) 32 hours ; (3) 20 hours ; (4) 8 hours. first period of therapy. (1) he was given a great deal of recognition and acceptance, and a very good rapport was quickly established between peter and the therapist. (2) an extensive mental hygiene progamme was carried out, orientated to his personal needs. his fears were discussed,' (a) fears of speaking situations, and (b) other fears. the therapist helped him work though many of his fears, mainly through discussion and assignments. his secondary symptoms were explained to him as means of trying to avoid, and thus "run away" from stuttering. as peter was such an intelligent boy, he got insight into his problems and mechanisms very quickly. (3) parallel with this mental hygiene programme, the therapist gave the case negative practice to alleviate his very severe secondary symptoms. the following procedure was carried out: (a) he recorded his speech and talked while looking into a mirror. this helped him to recognise and identify his secondary symptoms. he was also required to describe them. (b) he then had to imitate his secondary symptoms while recording and looking in the mirror. he then listened back to the recordings and compared his imitations to the real stutter. (c) when his imitations were exact replicas of his stutter, he recorded his reading while using the secondary symptoms before (i) every word, then (ii) every other r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) o u r n a l o f s o u t h a f r i c a n l o g o p e d i c s o c i e t y a r l word, then (iii) only words beginning with certain letters, then (iv) at the beginning of sentences, then (v) at the beginnings of sentences in certain paragraphs. peter was required to read chorally with every recording of his readings that he had made, using the secondary symptoms where he had used them in the recorded passages. (d) exactly the same procedure was carried out while he was conversing, except that while he was speaking with the recorder, he looked in the mirror as well. he spent approximately two to three hours a day on negative practice while he was receiving therapy. when he left johannesburg he was told to continue with it for thirty minutes a day, and to use it sometimes when talking to his parents and friends. the therapist advised him to be careful not to develop new stuttering symptoms, and to treat them by using negative practice if they did occur the results of the first period of therapy were very dramatic, and indicated that this case had benefitted a great deal from the parallel approach to attitude and secondary symptoms. recordings, when compared with the first ones showed a marked improvement, there were considerably fewer blocks, and the speech was much more intelligible. the 2nd, 3rd and 4th periods of therapy. therapy was continued along the same lines as that of the first period. he was also using many secondary symptoms in dramatised and later, real life situations. if, while using this controlled speech, a secondary symptom "slipped out" he had to say the whole sentence again as a punishment. at the end of each period of therapy he was told to practise at home. he did this very conscientiously and his mother helped him a great deal. results of therapy. (1) on the neurotic scale he scored average. (2) attitude scale found a great improvement in the way he felt about his speech. (3) severity of stuttering, he scored "4 average : stuttering on about 5 to 8 per cent, of words; tension occasionally distracting ; blocks average about one second , stuttering pattern characterized by an occasional complicating sound of facial grimace; an occasional distracting associated movement." his parents found him much more self-confident than before. he progressed very favourably at school, and he was no longer afraid to speak. conclusions. this case clearly demonstrated the advisability of a parallel approach to stuttering: utilising the technique of negative practice, and carrying out a mental hygiene programme concurrently. i have found these therapies most successful in the cases of adolescents with severe secondary symptoms and bad attitudes towards their stuttering. the attitude approach helps them to overcome some of their anxiety, and mental tension, while they feel that they are helping themselves by carrying out a speech programme. i have found that negative practice is most successful when carried out on an intensive level, otherwise the case is inclined to tire of it and become disheartened. negative practice is a difficult technique, and must be presented to the case as a challenge. when he sees it as a challenge, he derives great satisfaction from its accomplishment. i should like to thank professor p. de v. pienaar, director of the university speech voice and hearing clinic, for permission to report on this case. the south african guild of speech teachers the secretary, 16, the braids road, emmarentia iohannesburg. phone 41-4337. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) abstract introduction the passive research method and design instruments, administration and scoring results discussion returning to the research questions conclusion acknowledgements references footnotes about the author(s) anneke p. potgieter department of general linguistics, stellenbosch university, south africa citation potgieter, a.p., (2016). lexical and grammatical development in trilingual speakers of isixhosa, english and afrikaans. south african journal of communication disorders, 63(2), a141. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i2.141 original research lexical and grammatical development in trilingual speakers of isixhosa, english and afrikaans anneke p. potgieter received: 28 aug. 2015; accepted: 09 feb. 2016; published: 20 may 2016 copyright: © 2016. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract background: there is a dearth of normative data on linguistic development among child speakers of southern african languages, especially in the case of the multilingual children who constitute the largest part of this population. this inevitably impacts on the accuracy of developmental assessments of such speakers. already negative lay opinion on the effect of early multilingualism on language development rates could be exacerbated by the lack of developmental data, ultimately affecting choices regarding home and school language policies. objectives: to establish whether trilinguals necessarily exhibit developmental delay when compared to monolinguals and, if so, whether this delay (1) occurs in terms of both lexical and grammatical development; and (2) in all three the trilinguals’ languages, regardless of input quantity. method: focusing on isixhosa, south african english and afrikaans, the study involved a comparison of 11 four-year-old developing trilinguals’ acquisition of vocabulary and passive constructions with that of 10 age-matched monolingual speakers of each language. results: the trilinguals proved to be monolingual-like in their lexical development in the language to which, on average, they had been exposed most over time, that is, isixhosa. no developmental delay was found in the trilinguals’ acquisition of passive constructions, regardless of the language of testing. conclusion: as previously found for bilingual development, necessarily reduced quantity of exposure does not hinder lexical development in the trilinguals’ input dominant language. the overall lack of delay in their acquisition of the passive is interpreted as possible evidence of cross-linguistic bootstrapping and support for early multilingual exposure. introduction background currently, half of the world’s children are estimated to be growing up learning two or more languages due to the nature of the home and/or community contexts in which they are being reared (grosjean, 2010). this phenomenon is especially common in asia and africa, including linguistically and culturally diverse south africa, where high population density in the many low socio-economic status (ses) areas increases cross-linguistic contact. despite the global pervasiveness of early multilingualism, there is a relative lack of studies on the effect of multilingual acquisition on linguistic development. this has led to divided opinions on the (dis)advantages of early multilingualism (montanari, 2010, p. 103). some argue that our ability for multilingual acquisition is part and parcel of our ‘human language making capacity’ (meisel, as cited in montanari, 2010, p. 103). common lay opinion, however, largely holds that children growing up in multilingual contexts will necessarily suffer developmental language delay – this perhaps on grounds of anecdotal reports of children showing a (initial) delay in vocabulary development when their two or more languages are considered separately, and perhaps on grounds of many parents’ conviction that exposure to more than one language ‘confuses’ a young child. objectives on empirical grounds, the study aims to inform the above debate, the outcome of which has serious implications for child language professionals, child rearing practices and language-in-education policies. the second aim of the study is to contribute to the as yet extremely limited pool of information on developmental norms for speakers of southern african languages, both monolingual and multilingual. without such norms, speech-language pathologists cannot perform accurate developmental assessments of the linguistic abilities of the majority of south african children. in addition, where monolingual norms are available, they are often, for lack of another option, used for the assessment of bilingual children. as a result, an increasing number of bilinguals are being mistakenly diagnosed with specific language impairment (sli) and word finding disorder, rather than credited as typically developing bilinguals who are likely to catch up to their monolingual peers, given sufficient time and exposure (cf. cost action is0804, n.d.; paradis, 2010; thordardottir, rothenberg, rivard & naves, 2006). this misdiagnosis is largely driven by the fact that bilingual children typically have a smaller vocabulary size in each language than monolinguals do (bialystok, luk, peets & yang, 2010). contribution to the field given the limitations of previous multilingualism research in that it focused mostly on bilinguals (cf., for example, blom, 2010; cornips & hulk, 2008; hoff et al., 2012; hulk & cornips, 2006; unsworth, 2007, 2008), the study reported on here investigates firstly the added effect of a third language in the simultaneous acquisition process. in contrast to the purely observational methodology of many previous studies on trilingualism, the present study employs experimental tasks for data collection and involves a detailed analysis of linguistic data. secondly, in investigating the simultaneous acquisition of isixhosa, south african english (hereafter ‘english’) and afrikaans, the focus is on a germanic-bantu combination of language families that has, to my knowledge, never been investigated in the context of early bilingual or trilingual language acquisition. finally, the present study allows for the testing of blom’s (2010) claim that when their weaker language in terms of exposure is considered, multilinguals will exhibit developmental delay, contrary to what the majority of studies on multilingual first language acquisition report regarding grammatical development in the input dominant language (cf., for example, genesee, 2001; meisel, 2001; nicoladis & genesee, 1997; paradis & genesee, 1996). the passive the acquisition of the passive according to deen (2011, p. 155), the passive might very well be the most widely researched grammatical construction in the field of child language acquisition due to the apparent delay in its acquisition. in the case of monolingual english children, research suggests that these learners generally take 5 years or longer to fully acquire the rules relating to passive constructions (baldie, 1976; demuth, moloi & machobane, 2010, p. 238; de villiers & de villiers, 1973). in the case of dutch (the language from which afrikaans largely derives), ‘hardly any’ uses of the passive have been noted in the speech of monolingual children of preschool age, that is, of 4 years and younger (gillis & de houwer, 1998, pp. 28, 35). surprising then is the spontaneous use of the passive in the speech of children as young as 3 years that has been reported in the case of southern african bantu languages, including isizulu (suzman, 1985, 1987, 1990) and sesotho (demuth, 1989, 1990; demuth et al., 2010); the eastern bantu languages kiswahili and kigiriama (alcock, rimba & newton, 2011); and north american inuit and mayan languages (demuth et al., 2010, p. 238). studies have shown that in those languages in which passive constructions are produced relatively early, adult speech generally exhibits a high percentage of such constructions (cf. deen, 2002, for kiswahili; alcock et al., 2011, for kigiriama; kline & demuth, 2008, for sesotho). isixhosa is similar to sesotho in terms of both language typology and the manner in which logical subjects may be questioned (the latter possibly increasing the frequency of passives – cf. demuth et al., 2010). as such, given the reported age of acquisition of the passive by sesotho monolinguals, it is highly likely that isixhosa passives too are acquired earlier than english and afrikaans passives in the case of monolingual children. by choosing to investigate trilingual participants who are acquiring a combination of languages in which the passive is acquired at different rates among monolinguals, an enquiry into the possibility of cross-linguistic grammatical bootstrapping is made possible. if at an age at which isixhosa monolinguals, but not yet english or afrikaans monolinguals, have typically acquired the passive, isixhosa–english–afrikaans-developing trilinguals perform on par with or better than english and afrikaans monolinguals, this would indicate that they may be using their more advanced knowledge of isixhosa passives to support the development of the passive in their other two languages. a brief cross-linguistic comparison the following brief description of passive constructions in the three languages of interest to this study will focus first on the two morphologically reduced germanic languages, english and afrikaans, before moving on to the highly agglutinating bantu language isixhosa. (for an in-depth comparative grammatical description of passives in these three languages, cf. potgieter, 2014). at the very least, the english and afrikaans passive verbal sequence contains a passive participle, that is, a non-finite verb which encodes passive voice, and a free morpheme in the form of a passive auxiliary. in the case of regular english verbs, the passive participle is derived by attaching a passive morpheme in the form of the -ed or -(e)n suffix to the verb stem (ouhalla, 1999, p. 170) as in the cat was stroked (by the boy). in the case of regular afrikaans verbs, this participle is derived by attaching the prefix geto the verb stem, as in die kat is (deur die seun) gestreel (the afrikaans translation of the english example). in english, the passive auxiliary be can take various forms (i.e. be, been, is and was/were) depending on the tense or aspect that is being expressed. in a similar fashion, the afrikaans passive auxiliary is phonetically realised as some form of wees (‘be’), that is, as wees, word, is or was. as illustrated in the examples above, an english passive sentence may optionally contain a by-phrase and an afrikaans passive sentence a deur-phrase, where the complement of the preposition by or deur thematically corresponds to the expression functioning as the subject in the active counterpart of the sentence. passives containing this type of phrase are often referred to as ‘long’ or ‘agentive’ passives, as opposed to ‘short’ or ‘agentless’ passives where the agent is left unspecified through the omission of this phrase. one function of the optional byor deur-phrase is to place emphasis on a ‘heavy/lengthy’ agent argument (ponelis, 1989, pp. 324–326). turning to isixhosa, a sentence is marked as expressing the passive voice through the use of a bound morpheme that is attached to the verb stem. this affix commonly takes one of two forms: -iwor -w-, the addition of the latter morpheme resulting in various (morpho-) phonological changes (louw & jubase, 1963, p. 111). contrary to english and afrikaans, isixhosa does not indicate tense by means of a free morpheme in the form of a passive auxiliary. the agglutinating nature of isixhosa verbal morphology renders the main verb finite in that tense is indicated by means of a specific affix on the verb itself. the affixes that mark the tense of passive isixhosa verbs are generally the same ones also found with active verbs, except in the case of the perfective, which uses two distinct markers. example (b) in (1) below illustrates the passive counterpart of the active sentence in (a): as shown in (1) above, isixhosa allows for long passives through the use of a copular noun phrase (here, ngujohn) introduced by a copular prefix which serves the same semantic function as the english preposition by and the afrikaans preposition deur in the context of passive sentences. the form of this prefix (here, ng-) is determined by the class of the noun to which it attaches (louw & jubase, 1963, p. 106). although the isixhosa copular noun phrase, like the english by-phrase, may occur only postverbally, its exact postverbal position may change in line with discourse factors (du plessis & visser, 1992, p. 84). like english and afrikaans, isixhosa allows expletive passive constructions in which the structural subject position is thematically empty due to the object argument (in isixhosa, regardless of it being definite or indefinite) remaining in its original position. in english and isixhosa, the latter is a postverbal position, both languages being underlyingly svo. in such constructions, the isixhosa passive verb takes the expletive prefix ku(du plessis & visser, 1992, p. 70). unlike in the other two languages, the structural subject position in isixhosa expletive passives is not filled by a free morpheme (such as there or daar) but is left phonetically empty. underlyingly, however, this position is filled by an existential pronominal element that is associated with ku(du plessis & visser, 1992, p. 72).2 an example of an isixhosa expletive passive is given in (2) below, the definite object argument being italicised: research questions given the objectives outlined in previous section, the primary research question that drove this study is: does trilingual exhibit developmental delay when compared to monolingual? if so: does this delay occur both in terms of lexical and grammatical development (the latter being gauged on grounds of knowledge of passive constructions)? does this delay occur in the case of all three languages, or only in the language(s) that are weaker in terms of quantity of input? research method and design participants a total of 41 four-year-old children were recruited for participation: 11 isixhosa-english-afrikaans developing trilinguals (mean age 54.1 months), 10 monolingual isixhosa controls (mean age 55.2 months), 10 monolingual english controls (mean age 54.1 months) and 10 monolingual afrikaans controls (mean age 51.2 months). seven of the trilinguals were female and the remainder male, with each monolingual group being equally divided in terms of gender. all participants qualified as typically developing on grounds of parents’ answers to those questions in a language background questionnaire (lbq) that enquired as to developmental milestones, a possible history of (periods of) hearing impairment and general concerns about the child’s linguistic development. all participants were from homes with low ses. (cf. potgieter & southwood, 2016, for more information on how ses was calculated and for an assessment of the suitability of the lexical measure used in the present study for use with low ses individuals). among prospective trilingual participants, an age of first exposure before 4 years in the case of each of the child’s three languages (cf. potgieter, 2014, for justification), as well as a reported ability to communicate meaningfully in all and only the three languages of interest to this study, were considered as selection criteria. as for the monolingual groups, the typical high level of sociolinguistic diversity of most low ses areas in the western cape made finding prospective participants with exclusive exposure to only one language nearly impossible. as such, participants were considered suitably monolingual if their parents reported that they are unable to speak and/or understand any language other than the one of interest to the extent that they can coherently converse in that language. also, if an additional language was spoken in the home, exposure had to be limited enough to prevent the child from being able to regularly and spontaneously produce words in that language. setting the trilingual participants were sourced from eight different crèches (i.e. day-care centres) situated in townships and low-income informal settlements. these crèches mostly serve children from a range of different racial, cultural and linguistic backgrounds. as such, their child populations consist predominantly of englishand afrikaans-speaking so-called coloured3 children; followed in number by isixhosa-speaking black children; and then black children of immigrant descent, speaking other indigenous african languages as first language (l1), with mostly english as second language (l2). the levels of multilingualism in these groups vary, but the majority of the children are at least functionally bilingual, knowing either english or afrikaans (to varying extents) as one of their languages. at the time of the study, the trilingual participants from these crèches were receiving exposure to at least two languages in the home environment and at least three in the community and/or crèche context (afrikaans and/or english being the primary media of instruction at the relevant crèches). the isixhosa monolinguals were recruited from three crèches in a particular township and informally from a number of homes in a low-income suburban area; the english monolinguals from three crèches in a low-income suburban area; and all the afrikaans monolinguals from the same crèche in a farmworker community. ethical considerations ethical clearance for the study was obtained from the research ethics committee: humanities (nhrec nr rec-050411-032) of stellenbosch university. the objectives and procedures to be followed in the study were discussed verbally and presented in writing (as an informed consent form) to the parents of the child participants. the lack of financial compensation for participation and any hazard or direct benefit to participants was clearly communicated. participants were assured that participation would be anonymous, voluntary and could be discontinued at any point. signed informed consent from the parents and assent from the children were obtained (children indicating such assent with an ‘x’). all data have been protected either in a locked cabinet or, in the case of electronic data, on a password-protected laptop. instruments, administration and scoring language input information regarding the trilingual participants’ language exposure was gathered by means of a specially designed parental lbq in conjunction with a teacher report (cf. potgieter, 2014, for copies of these instruments) and analysed using the utrecht bilingual language exposure calculator (ubilec; unsworth, 2011a, 2011b, 2013). the lbq mainly enquires as to the child’s language exposure over time and at the time of testing, within various contexts, and includes a section requiring an hour-by-hour description of a typical day in the child’s life (both during the week and over weekends), with specific information on the languages used in each context and their relative distribution. to control for varying literacy levels among the low ses respondents, the lbq was administered as an oral interview conducted, in the case of the trilinguals, in the respondents’ homes and in the case of the monolinguals, mostly telephonically. the respondents’ l1 was employed in all cases, using a cultural and linguistic broker where necessary. all interviews were audio recorded, transcribed and (where necessary) translated. the results of teachers’ reports on the purpose and distribution of various languages in the crèche context were used to verify or correct parents’ reports on this matter. the ubilec (unsworth, 2011a, 2011b, 2013) consists of an lbq and an accompanying microsoft excel spreadsheet that quantifies the collected exposure data, calculating values for the following four variables in the case of each language: (1) current amount of exposure (caoe) as percentage of the child’s waking hours in a typical week; (2) cumulative length of exposure (cloe) in years, accounting for varying amounts of exposure over time; (3) traditional length of exposure in years (tloe), that is, age at testing minus age at onset of acquisition; and (4) quality of current input (in terms of ‘nativeness’, measured on a scale of zero to five). although the regular ubilec lbq was not employed in the present study, the specially designed lbq elicited the necessary degree of detail concerning language exposure to render its results suitable for quantification via the ubilec spreadsheet. lexical measure the vocabulary tests employed in this study are the language impairment in a multilingual society: cross-linguistic lexical tasks-isixhosa (litmus-clt-xho; southwood & potgieter, 2013), cross-south african english (litmus-clt-sae; southwood, 2012b) and cross-afrikaans (litmus-clt-af; southwood, 2012a). the aim of the research action behind the design of these instruments, and the 31 other language versions, was to disentangle bilingualism and sli and to profile bilingual sli in children from bilingual migrant communities in europe (cost action is0804, n.d.). as such, the design of these instruments enables the fully comparable assessment of lexical ability across a multilingual child’s different languages (cf. haman, łuniewska & pomiechowska, 2015). the litmus-clts consist of four sections: noun production, verb production, noun comprehension and verb comprehension. the comprehension sections constitute a picture-selection task and the production sections a picture-naming task. testing was conducted in as quiet a room as possible on the crèche premises, except in the case of the few isixhosa monolingual participants who did not attend the crèche and had to be tested at home. the trilingual participants were tested three times (once in each of their languages), with a week in between each testing. this period was deemed sufficient to minimise the chance of practice/priming effects. in addition, every effort was made to counterbalance the order of the language tests between children, taking into account the availability of assistants and participants, as well as crèche schedules. practice/priming effects are, however, unlikely, given that the different language versions of the litmus-clts differ in their target word selection, the difficulty rating of words being the variable that was controlled for in their design. in the case of the monolingual participants, a single testing session for each child was necessary. the order in which the four sections of the litmus-clts were administered was counterbalanced within each language group in the case of both the trilinguals and monolinguals. in the scoring of the production items, the soft score option (crediting regional variants and synonyms) was employed. see potgieter and southwood (2016) for more information on this decision, as well as some of the difficulties surrounding the scoring of responses to the isixhosa version of this instrument. grammatical measure the instrument employed to test the acquisition of passive constructions is a subsection of southwood and van dulm’s (2012a, 2012b, 2013) language therapy instrument known as receptive and expressive activities for language therapy (realt). this instrument was designed to enhance the language intervention process in the case of l1 and l2 englishand afrikaans-speaking children with sli or a language delay/disorder stemming from some other condition (southwood & van dulm, 2012b, p. 1) and has since also been translated into isixhosa. included in the target population of this instrument are children from low ses communities whose general and classroom-relevant linguistic skills may be developed through the type of language stimulation that the use of this instrument can offer (southwood & van dulm, 2012b, p. 1). the passives subset of the realt tests both the comprehension (by means of a picture-selection task) and production (by means of a sentence-completion task) of a number of different types of passive constructions, that is, short passives, long passives, actional passives, expletive passives and reversible long passives (i.e. long passives in which the expression denoting the animate agent argument and that denoting the animate theme argument are interchangeable, even if such an alteration renders the interpretation somewhat improbable, for example, the cat was chased by the dog versus the dog was chased by the cat). in each testing session, the comprehension and production halves of the passive test were interchanged with the two halves of the vocabulary test to prevent the child from becoming bored with either test. as the realt was not designed to serve as a formal test instrument but as language therapy material, the researcher developed her own scoring system: a mark of one was awarded for every answer in the comprehension section that corresponded with the number of the target item, and a mark of zero for every incorrect answer. as for the production items, the child’s verbatim response was entered in a spreadsheet and a score of zero, one or two awarded, depending on the extent to which the response approached or deviated from the target answer. in short, an exactly on-target passive sentence or accurate passive sentence using a different yet suited passive verb was awarded two marks; a passive sentence with a morphological error on the verb or a long passive sentence that is on target except for an error in the agentive phrase was awarded a mark of one, and any answer that does not constitute a passive sentence was awarded a mark of zero. see potgieter (2014) for a more complete description of the scoring process and the justification behind it. results results of the trilingual group’s language exposure measures table 1 presents, in the form of descriptive statistics, the results of the quantification of the trilinguals’ exposure data through means of the ubilec excel spreadsheet. for the purposes of the present study, only the variables of caoe and cloe are reported on. to enable a comparison of cloe across children of slightly varying ages (i.e. between 4.00 and 4.99 years), cloe in years was recalculated to present a percentage portion of the child’s age in years. results revealed that, at the time of testing, the majority of the trilinguals’ exposure was, on average, in the medium of english at 49.1%, followed by isixhosa at 34% and then significantly less afrikaans at 16.6%. however, by far the majority of the cumulative exposure that these children were exposed to over time took the form of isixhosa at an average of 58.2%, with english and afrikaans trailing behind at around 19% each. table 1: trilingual group’s (n = 11) language exposure data, as percentage. results of the lexical measure descriptive statistics the trilingual and different monolingual groups’ scores on the three different language versions of the litmus-clt were not normally distributed; hence, median and iqr (rather than mean and range) have been used in the reporting of these data in table 2. table 2: monolinguals’ (n = 10 per language group) versus trilinguals’ (n = 11) median scores on the litmus-clts, as percentages with iqr below it. across all three language groups, in both the monolingual and trilingual data, the total scores for comprehension are higher than the total scores for production, for example, 67.97% vs 35.94% in the isixhosa monolingual data and 62.5% vs 34.38% in the isixhosa trilingual data. this lag between the development of comprehension and production skills has been widely reported in the literature on child language acquisition and has been found to exist across many languages among both monolinguals and bilinguals. see, for example, benedict (1979); harrisa, yeelesa, chasina and oakley (1995); windsor and kohnert (2004); and for studies on the acquisition of grammatical agreement by monolingual isixhosa speakers, gxilishe, smouse, xhalisa and de villiers (2009) and smouse, gxilishe, de villiers and de villiers (2012). also note that the scores on the noun sections are consistently higher than the scores on the verb sections. consider, for example, the isixhosa monolinguals’ noun total of 59.38% versus their verb total of 48.44% and the isixhosa trilinguals’ noun total of 56.5% versus their verb total of 43.75%. these data align with a large body of studies that has shown the acquisition of nouns to precede the acquisition of other lexical categories across many languages, with some studies using bilingual participants (cf. chan & nicoladis, 2010, for references to numerous relevant studies). comparison across monolingual groups the kruskal–wallis (non-parametric) anova test was used to test for significant differences between the three monolingual groups’ overall test scores, total comprehension scores and total production scores ( p < 0.05 qualifying as significant). results revealed a significant difference between the three language groups in terms of their overall test scores (h(2) = 9.11, p = 0.01). bonferroni-adjusted post hoc tests revealed this to be due to the overall scores of the isixhosa group (mdn = 53.1) being significantly lower than the overall scores of the english group (mdn = 67.6), p = 0.049, and the afrikaans group (mdn = 75.4), p = 0.02. a second significant difference between the three language groups is found in the case of the total production scores (h(2) = 15.09, p < 0.01). this is again due to the isixhosa group’s total production scores (mdn = 35.9) being significantly lower than those of the english group (mdn = 54.7), p < 0.01, and those of the afrikaans group (mdn = 71.9), p < 0.01. in terms of total comprehension scores, however, the english (mdn = 80.5), afrikaans (mdn = 79.7) and isixhosa (mdn = 68) monolingual groups do not differ from one another significantly (h(2) = 4.39, p = 0.11). this overall pattern of results is also reflected in participants’ scores when the respective sections of the litmus-clts that test knowledge of verbs and nouns are considered in their own right, rather than subsumed under the overall score or total comprehension/production scores (cf. the descriptive statistics in table 2). comparison between monolinguals and trilinguals the non-parametric mann–whitney u test was used to test for significant differences between, in the case of each of the three languages, the monolingual and trilingual participants’ overall test scores, total comprehension scores and total production scores. in the case of both english and afrikaans, the trilingual group (mdns: english = 43.8, afrikaans = 31.3) scored significantly lower than the monolingual groups (mdns: english = 67.6, afrikaans = 75.4) on the test as a whole (english: z = −3.38, u = 6.5, p < 0.01; afrikaans: z = −3.63, u = 3; p < 0.01). on the comprehension sections of the english and afrikaans tests too, the trilingual group (mdns: english = 62.5, afrikaans = 54.7) was significantly outperformed (english: z = −2.36, u = 21, p = 0.02; afrikaans: z = −2.92, u = 13, p < 0.01) by the monolingual groups (mdns: english = 80.5, afrikaans = 79.7). finally, this same pattern is found in the case of the production sections (english: z = −3.45, u = 5.5, p < 0.01; afrikaans: z = −3.27, u = 8, p < 0.01), with the trilinguals (mdns: english = 20.3, afrikaans = 18.8) faring significantly worse than the monolingual groups (mdns: english = 54.7, afrikaans = 71.9). in the case of isixhosa, however, there were no significant differences between the trilingual and monolingual groups’ performance on any of the three compared measures, that is, not in terms of their overall test scores (z = −0.67, u = 45; p = 0.5), total comprehension scores (z = −0.67, u = 45; p = 0.5) or total production scores (z = −0.49, u = 47.5, p = 0.62). this overall pattern of results is again also reflected in participants’ scores on the verb and noun subsections. results of the grammatical measure descriptive statistics medians and iqrs are again applicable to the non-normally distributed scores on the passives subset of the three relevant language versions of the realt, reported in table 3. table 3: monolinguals’ (n = 10 per language group) versus trilinguals’ (n = 11) median scores on the realt, as percentages with iqr below it. note firstly that, as in the case of the lexical measure, all participant groups generally have lower scores for production items than comprehension items. consider, for example, the english monolinguals’ total production score of 2% versus their total comprehension score of 52.9% and the english trilinguals’ total production score of 0% versus their total comprehension score of 45.7%. paradis (2010, p. 675) points out that production tasks are even more demanding for bilingual children than for monolingual children and cautions that such tasks may therefore produce an inaccurately poorer picture of bilinguals’ knowledge of a specific structure than is actually the case. secondly, note that in the case of the trilinguals’ performance in each language, the median score for the comprehension section as a whole is close to 50%, the iqrs also being limited to no lower than 7% and no higher than 10% above 50%. the comprehension sections of the realt present the child with a choice between three pictures – one picture being the target, one the opposer and the other the distractor. as such, a score of 33% for comprehension may be said to represent chance level. in order to ensure that the trilinguals’ scores for comprehension in each language, despite being relatively low, are still significantly higher than the chance level, a sample t-test was run on these data. results confirmed that this was indeed the case for the isixhosa (t(10) = 8.39, p < 0.01), english (t(10) = 4.72, p < 0.01) and afrikaans (t(10) = 7.02, p < 0.01) data. comparison across monolingual groups care was taken in the design of the realt to ensure that each test item, across the different language versions, targets the same structure and is comparable in terms of its degree of difficulty (southwood & van dulm, 2012b, p. 4). however, this instrument was not designed with a statistical across-language comparison of results in mind; the statistical analysis reported below should therefore be interpreted with caution. the kruskal–wallis non-parametric anova test was used to test for, in the case of overall test scores and comprehension scores, significant differences between the three monolingual groups. because the english and afrikaans production data evidently suffered from floor effects (the median scores for all but one of the production subsections being 0%), these data were not deemed fit for statistical analysis. as such, the total production scores were compared in a descriptive fashion only. the anova tests revealed a significant difference between the three language groups in terms of their overall test scores (h(2) = 7.55, p = 0.02). bonferroni-adjusted post hoc tests revealed this to be due to the total scores of the isixhosa group (mdn = 50.4), being significantly higher than the total scores of the afrikaans group (mdn = 30), p = 0.02. this significant difference in terms of overall test scores must be a result of differences in terms of production as the isixhosa, english and afrikaans monolingual groups do not differ significantly from one another in terms of their total comprehension scores (h(2) = 0.86, p = 0.65), their scores for the comprehension of long actional passives (h(2) = 5.5, p = 0.06) or their scores for the comprehension of short actional passives (h(2) = 0.11, p = 0.95). recall that in the case of the litmus-clt data too, the three monolingual groups did not differ significantly in terms of comprehension skills. it is clear from the raw median scores on the realt, however, that the english and afrikaans monolinguals fared much worse than the isixhosa monolinguals in terms of their total production scores and their scores on all the production subsections. comparison between monolinguals and trilinguals the participants’ percentage scores on the following test variables were statistically compared, using the mann–whitney u test: overall scores, total comprehension scores and scores for the comprehension of long and short actional passives, respectively. no significant differences were found in the case of any of the three languages (all u-values < 55 and all p-values > 0.17). total production scores were again compared in a descriptive fashion only, using raw median scores. these scores were highly similar across the trilingual and relevant monolingual groups, that is, 48% vs 51% in the case of isixhosa, 0% vs 2% in the case of english and 0% across both groups in the case of afrikaans. discussion the results of the study are discussed below in the same order as which they were presented in above. to conclude the discussion, the research question (and sub-questions) presented in previous section are answered. results of the lexical measure comparison across monolingual groups the 4-year-old english and afrikaans monolinguals in this study are on par with one another in terms of lexical development, but their isixhosa monolingual counterparts seem to have significantly lower (at least productive) vocabulary skills. possible explanations include: (1) a perhaps inevitable slight imbalance in the degree of difficulty of the litmus-clt-xho and that of the other two language versions; (2) culture-related differences between child-rearing practices and child-socialisation styles in black african versus so-called cape-coloured groups (the english and afrikaans monolinguals belonging to the latter cultural group and the isixhosa monolinguals to the former); and (3) despite a shared ses level, possibly lower print exposure among the ‘deep’ township isixhosa monolinguals than among the english monolinguals from more suburban areas and among the afrikaans monolinguals who live on farms but attend a crèche that has a small library. comparison between monolinguals and trilinguals in terms of overall test scores, comprehension scores and production scores, the trilingual group fared significantly worse than the monolinguals on the english and afrikaans tests but performed on par with the isixhosa monolinguals. these findings are not surprising when considering the differing amounts of exposure that the trilingual versus monolingual groups have had to the respective languages. on average, the trilinguals have a cloe to english that equates to only 19% of their lifetimes and their caoe per week to english amounts to an average of not more than 49%. in the case of afrikaans, the trilingual group’s average cloe equates to only 18.4% of their lifetimes, and their average caoe is only 16.6%. it is therefore not surprising that the trilingual group was consistently outperformed by the english and afrikaans monolingual groups, who have a cloe and caoe to the respective languages of close to 100%. the trilingual group average for cloe to isixhosa equates to 58.2% of their lifetimes and their caoe to isixhosa to 34%. this means that, with a cloe to isixhosa that is roughly three times that of their cloe to other languages, but still nearly 40% less than that of monolinguals, the trilingual group was able to keep pace with their monolingual isixhosa counterparts. thus, the trilinguals in this study do show lexical developmental delay when compared to age-matched monolinguals, but only in the two languages that are weakest in terms of input quantity (measured in terms of cloe), despite these trilinguals’ exposure to their strongest language also being significantly less than what monolinguals are privy to. results of the grammatical measure comparison across monolingual groups recall that there was no statistically significant difference between the three monolingual groups in terms of their comprehension of passives, but that (on grounds of descriptive analysis) the english and afrikaans monolinguals clearly fared much worse than the isixhosa monolinguals in terms of production. this constitutes the exact opposite pattern of that found in the monolingual litmus-clt data, in which the isixhosa group was consistently outperformed by the english and/or afrikaans group. let us assume that the significantly lower litmus-clt scores among isixhosa monolinguals are not a result of an imbalance in difficulty rating between the different language versions of the test. the opposing production patterns in the three monolingual groups’ lexical and grammatical test data may then be argued to indicate that, even if cultural child-rearing practices perhaps lower the quantity of child-directed input that the isixhosa monolinguals receive, the negative effect of lowered quantity of exposure on the acquisition of passive constructions is cancelled out by the positive effect of the (assumed) higher frequency of such constructions in the isixhosa child-directed speech that these children do receive. recall from a previous section that the ability to produce passive constructions seems to emerge in english and dutch monolinguals only after the preschool years, whilst the context-appropriate spontaneous use of the passive voice in the speech of children as young as three years has been reported for a number of other southern and also eastern bantu languages (cf. alcock et al., 2011; demuth, 1989, 1990; demuth et al., 2010; suzman, 1985, 1987, 1990). hence, the fact that the monolingual isixhosa 4-year-olds in the present study seem to be more capable of producing passives than english and afrikaans monolinguals is not surprising. comparison between monolinguals and trilinguals the low median scores for both the trilingual and monolingual groups across languages, especially in the case of production, indicate the high degree of difficulty that passive constructions pose to the 4-year-old participants in this study (cf. table 2). importantly, however, despite receiving less exposure to all three of their languages, the trilingual group is managing to keep pace with the monolingual groups. this was shown statistically for comprehension and seems similar for production when raw median scores are considered. recall that, in the case of the afrikaans and english versions of the litmus-clt, the trilinguals scored significantly lower than the monolinguals. it thus seems plausible that, at least as far as comprehension is concerned, the trilingual participants are transferring grammatical knowledge of passive constructions in isixhosa (obtained through greater exposure to this language over time, perhaps coupled with the higher frequency of passives in southern bantu languages) to their knowledge of english and afrikaans. if so, this would constitute a case of cross-linguistic grammatical bootstrapping, confirming that the assumed relatively high frequency of passive constructions in the input that a developing trilingual receives in one of her three languages (here, isixhosa) can enhance its acquisition in her other two languages (here, afrikaans and english).4 considering the low median scores on the english and afrikaans production sections, both the trilinguals and monolinguals scoring 0% on almost all subsections, it is evident that in the case of these two languages, the average four year old from a low ses context, even if monolingual, cannot yet produce passives. the bootstrapping effect mentioned above thus seems to be limited (at least at this stage in the trilinguals’ language development) to the comprehension of passives. returning to the research questions in light of the statistical comparison of the trilingual and monolingual groups’ test scores in each of the three languages and assuming that a significant difference indicates developmental delay, the simple answer to the question ‘do trilinguals exhibit developmental delay when compared to monolinguals?’ is ‘yes, but only in certain respects’. recall that sub-question (a) asked whether the developmental delay occurs both in terms of lexical and grammatical development. results showed it to occur only in in the case of lexical development (in as far as lexical proficiency can be assessed by a vocabulary test) and not in the case of grammatical development (in as far as knowledge of passives may be taken as an indication of grammatical proficiency). importantly, this lexical delay among trilinguals is seen to occur in the case of both production and comprehension across both nouns and verbs. given the floor effects in both the monolinguals’ and trilinguals’ english and afrikaans realt production data, the reported absence of a delay in the trilinguals’ grammatical development is based on a statistical comparison of comprehension data only, although the raw production data show a similar trend. the fact that the trilinguals do not exhibit developmental delay with regard to passives in any one of their three languages is an important, unexpected finding in light of the literature on bilingualism showing grammatical developmental delay to commonly occur in the case of the language of less exposure (cf., for example, blom, 2010; paradis, nicoladis, crago & genesee, 2010). for a discussion of the differential effect of input on lexical versus grammatical development found in this study, see potgieter (under review). sub-question (b) enquired as to whether the developmental delay among trilinguals manifests itself in all three languages or only in the language(s) that is weaker in terms of quantity of input. the answer here is that the developmental delay is only found in the case of the two languages to which the trilingual group, on average, received the least exposure over time, that is, english and afrikaans. when the average amount of input that the trilingual group was receiving at the time of testing (i.e. caoe rather than cloe) is considered, the reason why there are significant differences between the trilinguals’ and monolinguals’ scores in the case of the english test, but not in the case of the isixhosa test, is less clear – the trilinguals had a higher average caoe to english compared to isixhosa (and afrikaans). afrikaans, however, provides a simpler picture of the relationship between input and lexical development in being the language to which the trilinguals received the least exposure both in terms of cloe and caoe, and also the one in which the trilinguals exhibit the largest developmental delay. overall, the results of the present study align with the findings of a multitude of bilingualism studies in which participants’ lexical development lagged behind that of monolinguals in the case of their language of less exposure (cf., for example, macleod, fabiano-smith, boegner-page & fontolliet, 2012; thordardottir, 2011; thordardottir & brandeker, 2010, in press). the results further support those of bilingualism research in proving again that necessarily reduced exposure does not hinder lexical development in the input dominant language (in terms of exposure over time). significantly, the present study has shown this to be the case even when input in the dominant language constitutes as little as 58.2% of multilingual participants’ overall language exposure over time (as opposed to the approximately 100% exposure monolinguals receive). conclusion the study has shown that, among low ses developing trilingual learners of isixhosa, english and afrikaans, necessarily reduced input does not lead to a delay in their acquisition of the passive in any of their three languages, or to a delay in lexical development in their input dominant language, isixhosa. the lack of any delay in their acquisition of the passive could be a result of cross-linguistic bootstrapping in the form of transfer of advanced knowledge of passives in one language to their knowledge of passives in the other two languages. this finding contributes theoretically to our understanding of the interaction between multilinguals’ different language systems. it is also of practical value in indicating that exposing a child to multiple languages from a young age may support the earlier development of certain features in the child’s languages. in addition, the study has shown that it is possible for young children exposed to as many as three languages to attain a monolingual-like vocabulary in one language plus a certain amount of vocabulary in an additional two languages, a benefit that monolinguals are not privy to. finally, in its assessment of both monolingual and trilingual speakers of isixhosa, south african english and afrikaans, the study has contributed to the limited pool of normative data on lexical and grammatical development among speakers of southern african languages. despite the use of a larger number of participants that is typical of trilingualism research, the generalisability of the results of the study is admittedly limited by this number. hopefully, this study has served to set the scene for larger scale research into multilingual language acquisition within the south african (and more broadly, the african) context. only on grounds of sufficient research we may increase the accuracy of developmental assessments of children speaking local languages and ultimately cultivate full public understanding (specifically among parents and teachers) that childhood multilingualism does not necessarily pose a developmental hindrance but definitely offers children a valuable sociolinguistic skill – a skill that increases a child’s communicative and cultural resources and, in doing so, breaks down barriers. acknowledgements competing interests this study was made possible by the financial assistance of the harry crossley foundation and the national research foundation of south africa. any opinions, findings, conclusions or recommendations expressed in this material are those of the author, and therefore, the funding agencies do not accept any liability in regard thereto. the author reports no conflicts of interest. references alcock, k.j., rimba, k., & newton, c.r.j.c. (2011). early production of the passive in two eastern bantu languages. first language, 32(4), 459–478. baldie, b.j. (1976). the acquisition of the passive voice. journal of child language, 3, 331–349. bencini, g.m.l., & valian, v.v. (2008). abstract sentence representations in 3-year-olds: evidence from language production and comprehension. journal of memory and language, 59, 97–113. benedict, h. (1979). early lexical development: comprehension and production. journal of child language, 6(2), 183–200. bialystok, e., luk, g., peets, k.f., & yang, s. (2010). receptive vocabulary differences in monolingual and bilingual children. bilingualism: language and cognition, 13(04), 525–531. blom, e. (2010). effects of input on the early grammatical development of bilingual children. international journal of bilingualism, 14(4), 422–446. brooks, p.j., & tomasello, m. (1999). young children learn to produce passives with nonce verbs. developmental psychology, 35(1), 29–44. chan, w.h., & nicoladis, e. (2010). predicting two mandarin-english bilingual children’s first 50 words: effects of frequency and relative exposure in the input. international journal of bilingualism, 14(2), 237–270. cornips, l., & hulk, a.c.j. (2008). factors of success and failure in the acquisition of grammatical gender in dutch. second language research, 14, 267–296. cost action is0804. (n.d.). bi-sli, language impairment in a multilingual society: linguistic patterns and the road to assessment. retrieved march 16, 2014, from http://www.bi-sli.org/index.html deen, k.u. (2002). the acquisition of nairobi swahili: the morphosyntax of inflectional prefixes and subjects. doctoral dissertation. los angeles, ca: university of california. deen, k.u. (2011). the acquisition of the passive. in j. de villiers & t. roeper (eds.), handbook of generative approaches to language acquisition (pp. 155–187). new york: springer. demuth, k. (1989). maturation and the acquisition of the sesotho passive. language, 65(1), 56–80. demuth, k. (1990). subject, topic and the sesotho passive. journal of child language, 17, 67–84. demuth, k., moloi, f., & machobane, m. (2010). 3-year-olds’ comprehension, production, and generalization of sesotho passives. cognition, 115, 238–51. de villiers, j. (1984). learning the passive from models: some contradictory data. paper presented at the 9th annual boston university conference on language development, boston, ma. de villiers, j., & de villiers, p. (1973). development of the use of word order in comprehension. journal of psycholinguistic research, 2, 331–341. du plessis, j.a., & visser, m. (1992). xhosa syntax. (1st edn.). pretoria: via afrika. genesee, f. (2001). bilingual first language acquisition: exploring the limits of the language faculty. annual review of applied linguistics, 21, 153–170. gillis, s., & de houwer, a. (1998). dutch child language: an overview. in s. gillis & a. de houwer (eds.), the acquisition of dutch (pp. 1–100). amsterdam: john benjamins. grosjean, f. (2010). bilingual: life and reality. cambridge, ma: harvard university press. gxilishe, s., smouse, m.r., xhalisa, t., & de villiers, j. (2009). children’s insensitivity to information from the target of agreement: the case of xhosa. in j. crawford, k. otaki, & m. takahashi (eds.), proceedings of the 3rd conference on generative approaches to language acquisition north america (galana) (pp. 46–53). somerville, ma: cascadilla proceedings project. haman, e., łuniewska, m., & pomiechowska, b. (2015). designing cross-linguistic lexical tasks (clts) for bilingual preschool children. in s. armon-lotem, j. de jong, & n. meir (eds.), methods for assessing multilingual children: disentangling bilingualism from language impairment (pp. 194–238). bristol, uk: multilingual matters. harrisa, m., yeelesa, c., chasina, j., & oakley, y. (1995). symmetries and asymmetries in early lexical comprehension and production. journal of child language, 22(1), 1–18. hoff, e., core, c., place, s., rumiche, r., señor, m., & parra, m. (2012). dual language exposure and early bilingual development. journal of child language, 39, 1–27. hulk, a., & cornips, l. (2006). the acquisition of definite determiners in child l2 dutch: problems with neuter gender nouns. in s. unsworth, t. parodi, a. sorace, & m. young-scholten (eds.), paths of development in l1 and l2 acquisition, (pp. 107–34). amsterdam: john benjamins. huttenlocher, j., vasilyeva, m., cymerman, e., & levine, s. (2002). language input and child syntax. cognitive psychology, 45(3), 337–374. huttenlocher, j., vasilyeva, m., & shimpi, p. (2004). syntactic priming in young children. journal of memory and language, 50(2), 182–195. kline, m., & demuth, k. (2008). structural priming, thematic roles, and the acquisition of the passive. paper presented at the 3rd conference on generative approaches to language acquisition north america (galana 2008), university of connecticut, storrs, ct. louw, j.a., & jubase, j. (1963). handboek van xhosa. johannesburg: bonapers beperk. macleod, a.a.n., fabiano-smith, l., boegner-page, s., & fontolliet, s. (2012). simultaneous bilingual language acquisition: the role of parental input on receptive vocabulary development. child language teaching and therapy, 29(1), 131–142. meisel, j.m. (2001). the simultaneous acquisition of two first languages: early differentiation and subsequent development of grammars. in j. cenoz & f. genesee (eds.), trends in bilingual acquisition, (pp. 11–41). amsterdam: john benjamins. montanari, s. (2010). translation equivalents and the emergence of multiple lexicons in early trilingual development. first language, 30(1), 102–125. nicoladis, e., & genesee, f. (1997). language development in preschool bilingual children. journal of speech language pathology and audiology, 21(4), 258–270. ouhalla, j. (1999). transformational grammar: from principles and parameters to minimalism. (2nd edn.). london: arnold. paradis, j. (2010). bilingual children’s acquisition of english verb morphology: effects of language exposure, structure complexity, and task type. language learning, 60(3), 651–680. paradis, j., & genesee, f. (1996). syntactic acquisition in bilingual children: autonomous or interdependent? studies in second language acquisition, 18, 1–25. paradis, j., nicoladis, e., crago, m., & genesee, f. (2010). bilingual children’s acquisition of the past tense: a usage-based approach. journal of child language, 38, 554–578. ponelis, f.a. (1989). sintaksis. in t.j.r. botha, f.a. ponelis, j.g.h. combrink, & f.f. odendaal (eds.), inleiding tot die afrikaanse taalkunde (2nd edn., pp. 255–357). pretoria: academica. potgieter, a.p. (2014). the role of input in the early trilingual acquisition of english, afrikaans and isixhosa. doctoral dissertation. stellenbosch: stellenbosch university. potgieter, a.p. (under review). the effect of input on lexical versus grammatical development in early trilingual speakers of isixhosa, south african english and afrikaans. journal of child language. potgieter, a.p., & southwood, f. (2016). a comparison of proficiency levels in 4-year-old monolingual and trilingual speakers of afrikaans, isixhosa and south african english across ses boundaries, using litmus-clt. clinical linguistics & phonetics, 30(2), 87–100. savage, c., lieven, e., theakston, a., & tomasello, m. (2003). testing the abstractness of children’s linguistic representations: lexical and structural priming of syntactic constructions in young children. developmental science, 6(5), 557–567. smouse, m., gxilishe, s., de villiers, j., & de villiers, p. (2012). children’s acquisition of subject markers in isixhosa. in p. larranaga & p. guijarro-fuentes (eds.), pronouns and clitics in early acquisition (pp. 209–236). berlin: mouton degruyter. southwood, f. (2012a). language impairment in a multilingual society: cross-linguistic lexical tasks-afrikaans (litmus-clt-af). a part of litmus cost is0804 battery. unpublished material. southwood, f. (2012b). language impairment in a multilingual society: cross-linguistic lexical tasks-south african english (litmus-clt-sae). a part of litmus cost is0804 battery. unpublished material. southwood, f., & potgieter, a. (2013). language impairment in a multilingual society: cross-linguistic lexical tasks-isixhosa (litmus-clt-xho). a part of litmus cost is0804 battery. unpublished material. southwood, f., & van dulm, o. (2012a). reseptiewe en ekspressiewe aktiwiteite vir later-ontwikkelende taalvaardighede (realt): passive booklet [receptive and expressive activities for language therapy]. johannesburg: jvr africa. southwood, f., & van dulm, o. (2012b). reseptiewe en ekspressiewe aktiwiteite vir later-ontwikkelende taalvaardighede (realt): user manual [receptive and expressive activities for language therapy]. johannesburg: jvr africa. southwood, f., & van dulm, o. (2013). receptive and expressive activities for language therapy (realt): isixhosa version. unpublished material (available from the authors). suzman, s.m. (1985). learning the passive in zulu. papers and reports on child language development, 24, 131–137. suzman, s.m. (1987). passives and prototypes in zulu children’s speech. african studies, 46, 241–254. suzman, s.m. (1990). language acquisition in zulu. doctoral dissertation. johannesburg, south africa: university of witwatersrand. thordardottir, e. (2011). the relationship between bilingual exposure and vocabulary development. international journal of bilingualism, 15(4), 426–445. thordardottir, e., & brandeker, m. (2010). the role of language exposure: bilingual performance on a nonword repetition task. paper presented at the 28th world congress of the international association of logopedics and phoniatry (ialp), athens, greece. thordardottir, e., & brandeker, m. (in press). language exposure in bilingual toddlers: nonword repetition and lexical tasks. thordardottir, e., rothenberg, a., rivard, m. e., & naves, r. (2006). bilingual assessment: can overall proficiency be estimated from separate measurement of two languages? journal of multilingual communication disorders, 4, 1–21. unsworth, s. (2007). age and input in early child bilingualism: the acquisition of grammatical gender in dutch. in a. belikova, l. meroni, & m. umeda (eds.), proceedings of the 2nd conference on generative approaches to language acquisition north america (galana) (pp. 448–458). somerville, ma: cascadilla press. unsworth, s. (2008). age and input in the acquisition of grammatical gender in dutch. second language research, 24(3), 365–395. unsworth, s. (2011a). utrecht bilingual language exposure calculator (ubilec). unpublished material. unsworth, s. (2011b). utrecht bilingual language exposure calculator (ubilec): questionnaire and notes on completing the excel file. unpublished material. unsworth, s. (2013). utrecht bilingual language exposure calculator (ubilec): excel spreadsheet. unpublished material. vasilyeva, m., huttenlocher, j., & waterfall, h. (2006). effects of language intervention on syntactic skill levels in preschoolers. developmental psychology, 42(1), 164–74. whitehurst, g., ironsmith, m., & goldfein, m. (1974). selective imitation of the passive construction through modeling. journal of experimental child psychology, 17, 288–302. windsor, j., & kohnert, k. (2004). the search for common ground: part i. lexical performance by linguistically diverse learners. journal of speech, language and hearing research, 47, 877–890. zeller, j. (2008). the subject marker in bantu as an antifocus marker. stellenbosch papers in linguistics, 38, 221–254. footnotes 1. on the copula status of ng-, cf. du plessis and visser (1992). 2. for (minimalist) generative analyses of the expletive construction in bantu languages, cf. zeller (2008). 3. in south africa, this term is used to refer to persons of mixed ethnic origin, this mixed ancestry having roots in two or more of the following areas/groups: europe, asia and various indigenous khoisan and bantu tribes. although this term is viewed as derogatory in certain contexts, no suitable alternative has unfortunately yet been established – hence the cautious use of the term here. the term ‘black children’ is used with similar caution to refer to children of bantu descent when specific ethnic/tribal affiliation is either irrelevant, or when wanting to refer to this group as a whole. 4. this type of implicit learning effect caused by the naturally or experimentally increased frequency of passive constructions in the input has already been reported to occur within a single language (rather than cross-linguistically). specifically, increasing the frequency of passives in the input that english monolingual participants received led to their earlier comprehension of passives (cf. bencini & valian, 2008; brooks & tomasello, 1999; huttenlocher, vasilyeva, cymerman & levine, 2002; huttenlocher, vasilyeva & shimpi, 2004; savage, lieven, theakston & tomasello, 2003) and to their earlier production of passives (cf. de villiers, 1984; vasilyeva, huttenlocher & waterfall, 2006; whitehurst, ironsmith & goldfein, 1974). journal of the south african logopedic society september the initiation of pharyngeal voice and early speech lessons in the laryngectomee s. bauman, m.a. certain established speech therapy procedures exist for the laryngectomized patient and are easily found in the literature which exists today (y). there does however, appear to be a need for more explicit details concerning the initiation of voice and early speech lessons. surgical removal of the vocal cords does not necessarily result in permanent aphonia. the human organism with its ability to adapt can, in most cases, readjust and produce, with the tissues of the pharynx, a fricative noise which can be modified to resemble voice. in some cases where trauma or necessary surgical procedures have eliminated a large amount of tissue at the back of the tongue and in the pharyngeal wall, so that an adequate closure is not possible, the production of the psuedo voice is difficult, if not impossible. for these patients an artificial aid is required. the mechanical or electrical larynx should be considered.3 with most other laryngectomeees the speech therapist attempts to teach the oesophageal voice method ( 0 v m) or the pharyngeal voice method ( p v m ) . in the 0 v μ air is swallowed, like food, and driven into the oesophagus. by putting pressure on the stomach muscles the air is forced back up and with throat constriction vibration is set up which resembles a belch. this sound is modified to become the new voice. in the p v m the air is swallowed deep into the pharynx, "locked" there, and then forced out. the resultant vibration is the beginning of the new voice. the 0 v μ is sometimes unacceptable to patients, especially women, because of the emphasis on assuming a kinaesthetic set similar to that required for belching. in addition, the early belches are fairly loud explosive sounds which seem somewhat uncontrollable and disquieting. in the ρ v μ it seems that the shorter passage of air, in and out, requires less time and effort and is therefore easier and quicker. the initial voice, although softer, appears to be smoother in quality, more even-flowing and continuous. finally it is less interrupted in phraseology. most patients master the p v m without difficulty.4 to aid them it is sometimes advisable to begin with explanations and activities to suggest the ο v μ (up to the point of the belch) in order to stimulate the mental image and kinaesthetic impression of the action required. (see stage v). whatever the technique attempted, it is not essential for the speech therapist to be able to demonstrate the new voice pattern. some authorities (5. ρ 15; 6. ρ 5.) believe this to be essential, but the writer has found it unnecessary. the use of aerated-sherbet mixtures is also a matter of some controversy. gaseous liquid intake is advocated by some speech therapists who maintain that it helps the patient to swallow air and then regurgitate it. for the p v m liquids appear to be dispensable. during practice sessions, the patient swallows a great deal of air, even to the extent of becoming uncomfortably flatulent. aerated liquids only too quickly hasten his discomfort. stages in the initiation of the pharyngeal voice method stage 1: the therapist should start with relaxation1 in order that the patient gets an understanding of (a) tension, force and pressure, (b) too flaccid a physical attitude, (c) optimal tonicity. this is the basis of therapy, as specific areas of tension in an overall relaxed body, are the keys to success. the generally relaxed physical attitude aids in attaining a relaxed state of mind, free from overanxiety and hampering fears. stage ii. the patient proceeds directly from the relaxation couch to relaxed walking. often a person can become quite tense sitting still and trying to relax, but many tensions disappear when the patient is walking along with a comfortable, easy stride, with arms free and swinging and breathing deeply. with a slight quickening of the pace it is easy to gulp in air (like a fish) and swallow it by forcing it right back past the pharynx. stage iii. ask the patient to hold his breath and feel tension in the region of the diaphragm. rather than use technical terms such as "tense your diaphragm", it is advisable for the therapist r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society ^september to say "feel tension here", and to indicate manually exactly where the tension is required. the case should get the kinaesthetic stimulation by feeling how the therapist herself stiffens the 'area and makes it tense. | now the therapist should put it to the patient that when the area of the diaphragm is tensed, some other part of the body becomes tense. by 'tensing the diaphragm the back of the throat is tensed and the back of the tongue is raised slightly. (partly reciprocal action and partly suggestion.) stage iv: the patient should copy the therapist's action of: (1) placing the tip of the tongue against the gums of the lower teeth, (2) keeping the centre of the tongue flat on the floor of the mouth, (3) and raising the back of the tongue to make a posterior closure (as for the 'k' sound.) this action is called "locking" and the case must be conditioned to take up this position quickly and efficiently, on demand 8 : he must take in air, swallow it into the back of the throat, hold his breath, feel tension in the area of the diaphragm and throat and immediately "lock." stage v. explain to the case how normal speech is produced:— (1) the air is breathed in through the mouth and nose and goes to the lungs. it then comes up from the lungs— (2) the vocal cords come together (almost in an unconscious way), (3) air passes through them and sets up vibrations. (4) these vibrations become voice. the laryngectomee has no vocal folds and the air which he breathes in, enters the hole in the throat and passes into the lungs and also comes out through the same opening in the throat. air taken in through the mouth and nose is swallowed down, as if to the stomach, just as food is swallowed into the stomach. the explanation of pharyngeal voice is as follows:— (1) swallowed air comes up the oesophagus to the back of the throat. (2) certain tissues are brought together by constriction at the back of the throat to take the place of the vocal cords. (3) the air will pass these approximating tissues and set up vibrations, (4) these vibrations result in voice. nelson (8 ρ 19) says that the air comes up to "vibrate against the fold of the oesophagus, scar tissue and bones of the throat, and the palate and other resonators." this is valuable information for the speech therapist, but need not be available to the patient. just as we approximate our vocal folds almost unconsciously, the laryngectomee will approximate areas at the back of the throat without being burdened with anatomical details which he cannot understand very well and which will confuse him and thereby make him nervous and anxious. the normal speaker is not aware of how he moves his vocal folds and therefore the laryngectomee should not be required to give his adaptation undue attention. a kinaesthetic understanding is more important than many detailed and scientific explanations. it is of the utmost importance that the therapist should let the patient find his own adaptation as regards the approximation of tissues for producing voice. stage vi. the co-ordinating phase: guide the patient as follows:— (1) walk briskly and easily. (2) gulp in air through the mouth, (3) swallow the air past the back of the mouth, (4) hold it there by tensing the diaphragm and throat. (5) "lock" very effectively. (6) push the air out immediately after the "lock". do not say "k" which is the position being held, but let the sound come out "ah." (the sound must be "triggered" off immediately after a very firm "lock".) (8 ρ 44). the resultant sound will be the voice which is to be developed. keep repeating this procedure many times, allowing short rests if the patient becomes very bloated from swallowing too much air. occasional failure to produce this voice is to be expected as the skill is only newly acquired and has yet to be firmly established and perfected. the patient should attempt to retain and recall the kinaesthetic impression of what he did when he produced an adequate sound and try to reproduce it over and over again. failure may be due to the exertion of too much diaphragm pressure or to an inadequately firm "lock." this procedure is repeated for as many lessons as required in order to enable the patient to produce voice on demand. once he can produce the sound (voice) on demand, he can dispense with the walking activity for by this time he is able to swallow air effectively and is not dependent on the inrush of air provided by the brisk walking. when seated the 1 aticnt should be in a comfortable chair with a back support. the legs should not be crossed. (8 ρ 46). by shaping the mouth the voice changes from a noise to recognizable vowel sounds. practice is then directed towards lengthening the vowel sounds. the patient then proceeds to practice diphthongs. the case can now be made aware of his ability to make buccal sounds. if possible, this ability r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 10 journal of the south african logopedic society should not be made clear to him until such time as he has learned to produce voice. patients who realise that they can communicate with buccal speech often perfect its use quite skilfully and then persist in using it, to the detriment of learning to use the new voice. the ability to produce sounds which are not dependent on voice suddenly widens the whole horizon of the patient. the unvoiced consonants and the vowels or diphthongs are blended together to produce the first nonsense words. similarly, voiced consonants and vowels or diphthongs are blended. care should be taken to ensure that the patient blends effectively from the start. blending should not be isolated drill but should be practiced through the introduction of meaningful words almost immediately. this provides strong motivation for the patient. from monosyllabic words the patient attempts two and three syllabled words. phrases and sentences should be introduced without delay. it is not uncommon to find a laryngectomee experiencing some difficulty with "i", "r", "m" and "n" sounds. these sounds should not be allowed to effect the patient unduly and he should not avoid these sounds. by attempting them to the best of his ability each time they confront him, he will find that they will "develop" in the speech pattern without much difficulty. the "h" sound is the only one which the laryngectomee does not master. the clinician should prepare the case for a possible increase in coughing when the speech activity first begins. this should disappear about a week after the introduction of voice activity. he may also feel discomfort from flatulence which is caused by the uneconomical loss of air into the stomach. this flatulence disappears as he becomes more proficient. any indications of voice from the trachea should be eliminated by decreasing general tension and the force and pressure on the diaphragm. (8, "). as soon as the patient is speaking in sentences, the clarity of the speech performance should be checked frequently by giving formal and informal intelligibility tests. the therapist alone, and then an audience, listen and watch the speaker (auditory and visual cues) and then listen without watching (auditory cues only). audibility and intellegibility are also checked over the telephone and from a playback on a tape recorder. (auditory cues only). when the laryngectomee has regained the art of producing voice and speech, he should reintroduce into his speaking activities all the auxilliary actions that accompany dynamic communication such as a facil expression, pleasant hand movements and short phrases which the normal person continually uses such as "oh, really", "is that so," "pardon", etc. throughout therapy and especially in the initial stages the patient should feel that the whole september procedure is easy, and because of their anatomical structure, perfectly natural and attainable. \ often the patient presents a psychological picture not unlike that of the dysphemic. social situations are feared and communication is avoided, because it is easier to withdraw than to watch the reactions of the auditors. they are responsive to the reactions of the listeners (whĉ hear this somewhat unfamiliar voice) in just the same way as the stutterer reacts when he sees the listener uncomfortable in the face of his nonfluency. the emotional reactions of the patient are not overcome as rapidly as speech is learned. this aspect of rehabilitation cannot be hurried. it resolves itself slowly when the patient learns to forget the past and brings hope and fortitude to the future. bibliography 1. b a n g s , jack, l. b i b l i o g r a p h y ; e s o p h a g e a l speech. journal of speech d i s o r d e r s . 1947. vol. 12 pp. 339-341. 2. anderson, john, o. b i b l i o g r a p h y on oesophageal speech. j o u r n a l of speech d i s o r d e r s . 1954. vol. 19, no. 1, pp. 70-72. 3. b a n g s , j . "speech after l a r y n g e c t o m y " . journal of speech d i s o r d e r s . vol. 11. no. 3. sept. 1946. 4. sawkins, john. "voice w i t h o u t a l a r y n x . " the medical p r e s s . a u g u s t , 1949. 5. sommerville, a. "speech t h e r a p y for t h e l a r y n g e c t o m e e . " south african l o g o p e d i c society congress e d i t i o n 1950. pp. 13-18. 6. t e a c h i n g oesophageal speech. n e w s b u l l e t i n of t h e college of speech t h e r a p i s t s . j u l y 1954. no. 46, pp. 4-8. 7. jacobson, edmund. p r o g r e s s i v e r e l a x a t i o n . u n i v e r s i t y of chicago p r e s s 1938. 2nd. edit. 8. n e l s o n , charles, r. p o s t l a r y n g e c t o m y speech. f u n k and w a g n a l l s co. n e w york, 1949. 9. marland, p. m. "a direct method of t e a c h i n g voice after total l a r y n g e c t o m y . " speech vol. 3, 1949. pp. 4-13. miss bessie dembo 54 wingate mansions cor. smit & nugget streets, hospital hill, johannesburg phone 44-0860 typing and roneoing undertaken, especially theses and students' notes. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) case history of an eighteen-year-old stutterer by joy bloch. b.a.log.(rand). an intelligent 18-year-old male stutterer, who has been receiving speech therapy at the university of the witwatersrand speech, voice and hearing clinic, for the past two years, was asked to write on his experiences as a stutterer. his response, in the writer's opinion, contains much valuable information, and appears to indicate many insights and a change of attitude. it is presented here (with his permission), in the hope that it will be of interest to therapists in this field. a brief outline of his case history, together with an outline of the type of therapy given, will be included to supplement and place in context his contribution. in order to preserve the case's anonymity he will be referred to as "peter". case history peter lives with his mother and his sister, who is six years older than he is. his father died when he was in his final school year. he started stuttering when he was about 2\, when he began forming sentences. he was fairly slow in starting to speak. other developmental milestones were normal. there is no history of stuttering in the family. he obtained a first class matriculation pass, and is at present repeating his first year as a medical student. he intends becoming a psychiatrist. he has had a variety of therapies at various times, including speech therapy, hypnotherapy and psychotherapy. he was first seen at the university speech clinic when he was six, and he received a short period of therapy. after this he received therapy in the school situation. he was interviewed again at the clinic when he was 14, but did not receive therapy. he returned to the clinic in may 1961, and for a year received both group and individual therapy. apparently he did not relate well in the group, and it was decided that he receive only individual therapy this year. in conjunction with speech therapy he is receiving psychiatric treatment. it is reported that he had severe guilt feelings on the death of his father, and tends to be dominated by his sister and his mother. he apparently sets high standards for himself. he has threatened to commit suicide on several occasions. an account of his reactions to therapy and his general behaviour pattern will be given later in this paper. description of symptom peter is a severe secondary stutterer. when he was seen at the clinic in may 1961, his stutter consisted of tense repetitions and non-vocalized blocks accompanied by secondary symptoms of lip tremor, pursing of the lips, constant head nodding and jerking, and clicking of the tongue. he kept eye-contact for a part of each block. according to the interviewing clinician: "he frequently gave up the speech attempt. this was his most characteristic method of handling severe blocks. at these times, when tension was extreme during the spasm, he would almost break down and cry, remain silent, or would say "leave it" or "it doesn't matter". when pressed to continue he would become almost catastrophic. his obvious anxiety at his stuttering manifested itself in such physical symptoms as blushing, sweating and constant movements." when first seen by the present clinician his stutter consisted of tense repetitions, drawing out of the end part of a word to carry him on to the next, and some non-voca^ lised blocks accompanied by somewhat bizarre secondary symptoms of head nodding, eye-blinking, tremor of lips and jaw, and facial grimacing. he frequently blushed during a block, and occasionally broke out into a sweat. he used starters such as "well", went back on words that ;he could not say, and on rare occasions, when he had a particularly severe block, tended to give up the speech attempt completely. he sounded as 2 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) though he was "running short of breath", his general speech pattern having a very breathless quality. he maintained a certain amount of eye-contact during a block. he reported tension in the stomach area when stuttering. therapy peter received therapy on similar lines from two different clinicians. his previous clinician reported many of the difficulties encountered by the writer. peter was resistant to therapy from the start. he appeared to be arrogant and was resentful of the clinic, since here he had to face his stuttering and himself as a stutterer. moreover, he was treated as a stutterer, and this seemed to increase his resentment. he maintained that he would never accept himself as a stutterer, as this was tantamount to an admission of failure on his part, as what he wanted was complete fluency. he blamed all his failures and inadequacies on his stuttering, including academic, social and emotional failures. initially the writer had difficulty in establishing an adequate working relationship with peter. he made several attempts to discontinue therapy, usually giving very immature reasons. he would become excited and emotional when his immature rationalisations were pointed out to him, and this served only to increase his aggression and resentment. gradually the writer came to accept him as he was, and indicate her acceptance of him by not responding to him as other people usually did, i.e. with aggression and pity. this seemed to be a turning point in therapy, as from then on peter became more co-operative, and less prone to bouts of extreme excitability and aggression. peter was subject to periods of extreme depression. when depressed, he would frequently j respond by becoming silent and looking down, or alternatively would become excited and start to shout. when shouting, his speech was often fluent. peter talked freely aiid frankly about his feelings, and would place the ultimate blame for his depression! on his stutter. an attempt was made to discuss with him his assets and liabilities. to him, all his assets (he was quite frank about these and maintained he was superior to others in every way except in speech) were outweighed by the liability of his stuttering. this attitude hampered attempts to modify his stuttering, in that in the therapy situation, knowing that he was regarded as a stutterer, he was quite willing to stutter "openly" without avoidances. however, he was extremely reluctant to do this in any situation outside the clinic, especially with people who were not aware of his stuttering. in the clinic situation peter was able to stutter easily, with relatively little struggle, and was able to modify his symptoms in the various ways suggested, always being aware of what he was doing, and being able to discriminate the form the modifications took. he "did not like" the easy, "open" stuttering as the "stutterings lasted longer" than those of his usual pattern. the lengths of the easy and tense blocks were timed with a stop watch, and although the average difference in duration was negligible, peter maintained that the easy blocks "felt longer". peter was often loath to use the taperecorder when working on his stutter, as it made his "greatest liability" even more real to him. although he used the tape recorder frequently, he always appeared upset when hearing the play-back, and when he was depressed would refuse to use the recorder at all. peter admitted to having a fear of talking on the telephone. this was discussed, and he eventually carried out several telephone assignments in the clinic situation only. he would not 'phone outsiders. he reported that he used the telephone at home only when necessary. as therapy progressed, a change became evident in his symptoms. he rarely gave up speech attempts completely. he eliminated the use of starters to a certain extent, and would carry through with his blocks until he was able to release the word. he was also able to maintain eye-contact for most of the time, except when having a particularly severe block. i would now like to present his contribution on: "my experiences as a stutterer." "it must be difficult, well nigh impossible for non-stutterers to imagine what it is like to be a stutterer. to be reluctant to enter a shop to buy a box of matches or to pick up december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) a telephone, to be loath to speak to members of the opposite sex — nonsense! speech is as natural as breathing, walking or eating — to be done automatically, without thought. as a stutterer, i will try to explain why a stutterer acts as he does and what he experiences due to his inability to converse freely on a purely physical level. stutterers are only bound to one another by the bond of their stutter and therefore there is no specific "stuttering-type". the following lines, while they might well be applicable to many stutterers, only refer specifically to myself and must not be taken to be typical of all stutterers. if stutterers all conformed to one mental attitude, treatment of stuttering would be a comparatively easy matter. however — "vive-la-difference!" as a stutterer i soon developed the "giant in chains" attitude. what would i not achieve if i had normal speech! i do not think that i might be but a mediocrity if i did speak fluently. as i cannot, as it were, prove my mediocrity by normal means of expression, and i am forced to take a back seat due to my stutter, the stutter is then blamed for all my failures. it is a convenient scapegoat. however, the stutter is not completely guiltless. to a very appreciable extent, it has governed my growth of personality besides affecting me on a physical level. due to the stutter, i have learned to converse practically in monosyllables and to say in a very few words almost what i intend to convey. brevity precludes excess stuttering! a certain sharpness of tongue results and because these few words can usually be said without stuttering, we find our "fettered gaint", the "false fluency" having gone to his head, ironically being labelled a "chatterbox". also in my case, trying to be noted not for how i speak, but for what i say, i do not exactly exercise tact in my speech. for one who has a speech difficulty, i manage quite well to make my likes and dislikes known in no mean manner. incidentally i am also quite a successful heckler of university lecturers, no mean achievement for a stutterer in a most competitive field! of course, due to my failure in person to person relationships, i have tried to make up for this shortcoming by constantly trying to prove my superiority, or at least my "uninferiority" in other fields of endeavour, e.g. sport. to this end i improved myself in these fields — for example becoming a good dancer — and thus have tried to show that i am "one of the boys" even if i am a stutterer. but to my mind, all of these assets do not outweigh the liability of my stutter. the stutter, in fact, has assumed dimensions out of all proportion to its actual place in my personality make-up. undoubtedly it has become the most important factor in my life and due to my inability to rid myself of it — for i feel that i do not need it any more — i spend much time in deep bouts of depression which alternate with forced gaiety. no stutterer wants to continue stuttering all his life. one outgrows the need for this escape mechanism from the responsibilities of everyday life — for what better escape is there than a speech defect which prevents the stutterer from speaking freely? i find i have outgrown the need to stutter, but i am "stuck with it". my frantic efforts to disguise, hide or loose it, often using misguided methods, only makes the stutter worse and i experience a deep sense of guilt in failing myself and the people interested in my welfare by not speaking fluently. i, as a stutterer, developed a deep sense of frustration and resentment against my fate and also an infinite propensity for self-pity. in an effort to hide my stutter i exhibited a strange mixture of arrogance and shyness. to most people i present the facade of a terrible "holier than thou" attitude of aloofness, my reasoning being rather to be thought "stuck up" than to reveal myself as a stutterer. i find it hard to believe that people can accept me together with my stutter and at times i feel apologetic for subjecting them to my stutter. sometimes i feel aggressive, as it were daring them to laugh at me, and i might add, being slightly disappointed when they do not! but i am also shy, as i am terrified of being found out as a stutterer and exposed to / laughter or, as happens more often, pity. for i have found that on the whole people are more sympathetic and understanding, which rather rankles, as i do not like being placed in a position whereby people have cause to exhibit sympathy. i would much rather be the dispenser of sympathy than the recipient. on many occasions i have preferred to be thought arrogant, a fool,. or simply mad, 2 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society 1 r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) rather than reveal the fact that i stuttered or that i stuttered badly, for in normal speech, not resorting to monosyllables, it is practically impossible to hide a stutter completely. also i have let people think a statement of mine means one thing, when i meant something totally different, rather than have to explain myself and stutter over the explanation. one recent example of the misunderstanding caused by my stutter happened at university. a girl in my class, whom i rather fancied, was wearing a low necked blouse with a star of david very much in evidence round her neck. referring to the medallion, i asked her pleasantly whether she was advertising. to my consternation she misunderstood the remark and rather than have to stutter the explanation — at that time she did not know that i stuttered — i left her with a totally erroneous impression of my harmless little remark. there are many such examples of times i have awkwardly kept silent whilst with people, being though a boor because of my taciturnity. i have also agreed with complete lies to escape the embarrassment and bother of "stutter-explaining". being "imperfect in speech", i have set myself a very high standard of "perfection" which even a superman would find taxing. every success is deprecated, but failures are magnified out of true proportion, and of course blamed on my stutter. what ingenuity is evidenced in blaming even moderate success on my stutter! if i had not stuttered the success would have been far greater. this preciosity is perhaps the most lasting and far-reaching effect of my stutter. perhaps i can learn to live with the physical fact of my stutter, but unless i revise my thinking very radically, psychologically i will never be able to make the most of my opportunities and become adjusted to the restrictions imposed by my handicap." in the writer's opinion, peter's paper reflects an intellectual insight and appreciation of his problems. it also seems to indicate a growing awareness of the need to face up to and accept the fact of his stuttering. once he has achieved this, the path of therapy should be easier. summary an intelligent 18-year-old male stutterer, who attends the speech clinic, university of the witwatersrand, was asked to write on his experiences as a stutterer. as his paper, in the writer's opinion, contains meny insights and a change of attitude, it was presented in the hope that it would be of interest to therapists in the field. an outline of his case hisory, together with a brief outline of the type of therapy given, were included to place his contribution in context. opsomming 'n intelligente, agtienjarige hakkelaar, wat by die spraakkliniek van die universiteit van die witwatersrand behandeling ontvang, is versoek om sy ondervindings as 'n hakkelaar te beskryf. aangesien sy beskrywing, volgens die opsteller se mening, -n insig in sy probleem en 'n verandering van houding toon, word dit aangebied met die hoop dat dit van belang mag wees vir terapeute op die gebied. 'n oorsig van sy gevalsgeskiedenis, tesame met 'n kort oorsig van die terapie toegepas, is ingesluit om 'n geheelbeeld van sy bydrae te gee. references. van riper, c. "speech correction — principles and methods". prentice-hall inc., new jersey. revised edition — 1954. booklet. "on stuttering and its treatment". report of a conference held at nassau, bahamas in jan. 1960. sponsored by the speech foundation of america. december, 1962 journal of the south african l o o p e d i c society r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) pdf file abstract introduction methods results discussion inter-rater reliability scoring criteria conclusion acknowledgements references appendix 1 about the author(s) mari viviers department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa alta kritzinger department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa bart vinck department of speech-language pathology and audiology, university of pretoria, south africa marien graham department of statistics, university of pretoria, south africa citation viviers, m., kritzinger, a., vinck, b., & graham, m. (2017). preliminary psychometric performance of the neonatal feeding assessment scale. south african journal of communication disorders, 64(1), a163. https://doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v64i1.163 original research preliminary psychometric performance of the neonatal feeding assessment scale mari viviers, alta kritzinger, bart vinck, marien graham received: 21 may 2016; accepted: 22 sept. 2016; published: 30 jan. 2017 copyright: © 2017. the author(s). licensee: aosis. this is an open access article distributed under the terms of the creative commons attribution license, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original work is properly cited. abstract objective: the objective was to determine the preliminary psychometric performance of a new clinical feeding scale to diagnose oropharyngeal dysphagia (opd) in neonates. methods: twenty neonates with a median gestational age of 35 weeks were evaluated using the neonatal feeding assessment scale (nfas) and modified barium swallow studies (mbss). the results were compared. results: nine of the 20 participants presented with opd on the nfas. comparison of the scale’s results with instrumental mbss indicated that all participants without opd were correctly excluded (100% sensitivity). the specificity was 78.6%, indicating that three participants were falsely identified with opd on the scale. inter-rater reliability was determined on 50% (n = 10) of the sample. substantial agreement (80%) was obtained between two raters in five of the six sections of the scale and on the diagnostic outcome. conclusion: the preliminary performance of the scale appears to be promising. a further validation study will take place. introduction in a developing country such as south africa there is a need for valid clinical assessment instruments for use by local speech-language therapists (slts) in neonatal dysphagia (viviers, kritzinger & vinck, 2016). such a need was also identified by botha and schoeman and indirectly implied in the south african practice guidelines for paediatric dysphagia, as no standardised clinical assessment instrument is recommended to use with neonates (botha & schoeman, 2011; sashla, 2011a). due to a lack of regulated service delivery and instrumental assessment equipment available for diagnosing dysphagia in the public healthcare sector, comprehensive clinical assessment may be even more important in developing countries such as south africa than in developed countries. a limited number of slts experienced in the administration and interpretation of modified barium swallow studies (mbss) or fiberoptic endoscopic evaluations of swallowing are practising in the public and private healthcare sectors. since objective assessment measures were encouraged there has been a rise in demand for mbss in the paediatric population, but inadequate radiology infrastructure remains a concern (hiorns & ryan, 2006). pados, park, estrem and awotwi (2016) found a lack of validated feeding assessment scales for infants younger than 6 months that are supported by high level evidence in a recent review. they concluded that the early feeding skills assessment instrument (efs) was one of the instruments that had some supportive psychometric development and testing in the neonatal population. however, no supportive data on the content validity offered by experts in the area of neonatal feeding for the efs has been published. two additional instruments with the most extensive psychometric testing are the neonatal oral motor assessment schema (nomas) (palmer, crawley & blanco, 1993) and the schedule for oral motor assessment (soma) (reilly, skuse & wolke, 2000), which focus on oral motor skills of the neonate and infant (pressman, 2010; rogers & arvedson, 2005). these two scales do not consider the impact of environmental and internal disruptions on the infant’s physiological subsystems and its resulting effects on the feeding process and mother–infant interaction. in comparison, the efs aimed to assess oral feeding readiness in a more holistic manner. it is thus recommended that a wider range of infant systems and feeding skills should be evaluated in a comprehensive neonatal clinical assessment instrument than was included in the discussed instruments. because neonatal dysphagia services are an important component of early intervention, an assessment instrument should incorporate the principles of family-centred developmentally appropriate care, an asset-based approach, team collaboration and evidence-based practice (asha, 2008; ensher & clark, 2009; gooding et al., 2011; saslha, 2011b; thoyre, shaker & pridham, 2005). as the parent’s first and enduring caregiving task after birth is to feed the infant, the primary caregiver should be central to the dysphagia assessment process. the value of parental description of the feeding difficulty and observation of a typical feeding routine between the mother and infant during clinical assessment may hold direct benefits for parental compliance during intervention. in contrast, during a mbss the parent may not be elete central to the assessment procedure. to respond to the need for a valid neonatal dysphagia assessment instrument for use in resource-constrained developing countries, the neonatal feeding assessment scale (nfas) was developed and approved, using expert collaboration through the delphi method (viviers et al., 2016). panel members agreed on a need for a validated nfas. south african panel members favoured a comprehensive instrument while international members contributed to evidence-based item inclusion and the use of an objective scoring system (viviers et al., 2016). clinical assessment will never replace the gold standard of mbss but may contribute significantly to complex clinical decision-making in neonatal dysphagia. the research question posed for the current study was, ‘what are the preliminary psychometric properties of the newly developed nfas to diagnose oropharyngeal dysphagia (opd)?’ methods aims and objectives the aim of the study was to determine the preliminary psychometric performance of the nfas to diagnose opd. the objectives were to determine the sensitivity, specificity and accuracy of the nfas in comparison to the mbss and to verify inter-rater reliability. design a comparative within-subject design (meline, 2010) was used to investigate the psychometric properties of the nfas by comparing the nfas and mbss results. participants neonates admitted to a 29-bed neonatal intensive care unit (nicu) at a tertiary academic hospital in gauteng province, south africa, were purposively selected. mothers were verbally informed of the study and through a brochure in english, setswana or afrikaans, the most prominent languages spoken in the city where the study was conducted. written or verbal (in case of illiterate participants) informed consent was obtained from all mothers. twenty neonates were selected. the participant inclusion criteria were that the neonate should have a high-risk status such as prematurity, low birth weight (lbw), exposure to hiv or another risk factor (e.g. craniofacial anomaly), predisposing the neonate to feeding and swallowing difficulties; be an in-patient in the nicu; be medically stable for assessment as determined by the treating physician; be within the age range of > 32 weeks gestational age to 4 months corrected age post-term at time of assessment. neonates younger than 32 weeks gestational age are expected to display feeding and sucking difficulties as a result of immaturity and are typically not fed orally; they were not included. participant characteristics are presented in table 1. table 1: participant characteristics (n = 20). according to table 1 the participants were born at a mean premature gestational age of 35.15 weeks (sd = 3.066). the mean birth weight of the participants was low, 2.17 kg (sd = 0.845) and the mean length of stay in the nicu was 6 days. additionally, the sample consisted of slightly more female participants (60%). other risk factors contributing to feeding difficulties were hiv exposure in utero or during delivery (30%, n = 6), respiratory distress syndrome (55%, n = 11) and hyperbilirubinaemia (55%, n = 11). prematurity (80%, n = 16) and lbw (85%, n = 17) were the most significant known risk factors for opd (pados et al., 2016). materials the newly developed feeding scale (nfas) and an mbss data collection form (based on arvedson & brodsky, 2002; hall, 2001; swigert, 2010) were used. the mbss form indicated the stages of swallowing (oral, pharyngeal and oesophageal stages), the presence or absence of any form of dysphagia, and penetration or aspiration in the pharyngeal stage. in addition, a parent interview schedule included pre-, periand postnatal information and a description of the feeding problem according to the parents (based on arvedson & brodsky, 2002; hall, 2001; swigert, 2010). medical records were used for additional information. the development and content of the nfas were discussed in a previous study (viviers et al., 2016). the item selection in the sections of the nfas was based on theoretical constructs related to neonatal and early infant feeding and the clinical assessment of feeding skills. the instrument relies on physiological observations of the infant during feeding, how infant state is influenced by feeding and how feeding may subsequently disrupt a regulated state in the infant with feeding difficulties and an associated display of stress cues (figure 1). figure 1: nfas sections and items. the mbss was performed using a fluoroscope (sysco 19” version multi diagnosteleva fd screening machine from philips, amsterdam, netherlands) with dvd recording capabilities. procedures clearance was obtained from the research ethics committee at the university and the medical ethics committee at the tertiary academic hospital where the study was conducted. the mothers of the participants were interviewed, medical files were reviewed, a clinical feeding assessment was conducted using the nfas and a mbss was performed. the mbss was conducted within 7 days of the clinical assessment. the interviews, medical file review and clinical feeding assessments were conducted by the first author, a qualified slt, and three graduate students in speech-language pathology. all data collectors were trained. training was provided in a 6-hour session on the content, administration and scoring of the nfas. after the training session each trainee was expected to accumulate four practice assessments before data collection was initiated. inter-rater reliability data was obtained for two of the four data collectors (excluding the first author) on 10 infants (50% of sample). two senior slts working at the hospital conducted the mbss while blinded to the infants’ feeding history and diagnostic outcome of the clinical assessments. because feeding is an integrated process, with infant responses in the different sections occurring simultaneously, the order in which sections of the nfas are completed may vary. a breastfeeding session was observed, or the mother was asked to prepare the bottle feed (expressed breast milk or formula) or supplemented breastfeeding with tube feeding if the infant was not fully breastfed. the complete data collection procedures for the nfas are presented in appendix a. scoring instructions for each section are indicated on the instrument. a binary yes or no system is used. the outcome of each section is a yes or no conclusion regarding the possible presence of opd. each section score is transferred to the last page of the instrument, where the overall diagnostic outcome of the assessment is calculated. when a score of three or more yes responses is obtained, the assessment outcome indicates that opd is likely to be present. at least one of the three yes responses required for reaching the final diagnosis of opd must either be obtained in section e or f (viviers et al., 2016). during the mbss a solution of barium sulphate was reconstituted by mixing the powder with the mother’s expressed breast milk or recommended formula. during fluoroscopy the pulsed mode with appropriate collimation was used to limit radiation exposure (hernanz-schulman, goske, bercha & strauss, 2011; scott, fujii, behrman & dillon, 2014). a nuk (bonn, germany) medicpro first choicetm 120 ml infant bottle with a medicprotm disposable thermoplastic elastomer (tpe) teat size 1 was used. participants were positioned with appropriate supported seating in a tumble forms 2 feeder seattm, warrenville, united states. data analysis frequency distributions were calculated for the nfas data. criterion validity was determined by calculating sensitivity (%) and specificity (%) scores based on the comparative data sets. sensitivity determines the probability of the presence of opd, whereas specificity reveals the probability that opd will truly be absent when using the nfas (dawson & trapp, 2004). positive predictive value (ppv) and negative predictive value (npv) indicate whether the nfas predicted the true positive and true negative diagnoses correctly (dawson & trapp, 2004). the higher the percentage score derived for ppv and npv calculations, the better and more valid the predictive ability of the instrument (dawson & trapp, 2004). cohen’s kappa with accompanying asymptotic standard error (ase) was used to investigate the inter-rater reliability coefficient, together with p-bar calculations for the results obtained by two independent raters. the interpretation of the inter-rater reliability calculations (kappa) according to dawson and trapp (2004) and landis and koch (1977) are provided in table 2. a kappa value of greater than 0.41 was considered a minimal reliability criterion (dawson & trapp, 2004). accuracy of agreement between the nfas and the mbss diagnosis of opd was also investigated. table 2: interpretation guidelines for kappa values for inter-rater reliability. results neonatal feeding assessment scale results the nfas was administered on a sample of 20 participants to determine preliminary psychometric properties. the clinical assessment results were compared to the mbss results to determine which participants presented with true opd. table 3 presents the data obtained from the nfas assessment. table 3: nfas results (n = 20). according to table 3 nine infants (45%) presented with opd on the nfas. positive identification of opd could be explained by the participant characteristics (see table 1) and the previously stated associated risk factors in the sample. as per scoring guidelines, the nine participants obtained a minimum score of three yes responses in the five sections, with one of the yes responses in either section e or f of the nfas. in sections c (stress cues during feeding) and f (clinical feeding and swallowing evaluation) the highest number of indicators were observed in those neonates diagnosed with opd. some of the neonates were not attached to heart rate and respiratory monitors. therefore certain items could not be scored in sections a and b (physiological status and state of alertness), resulting in low scores in the combined section. as a result of the low scores in the physiological status and state of alertness sections, the contributions of these sections to diagnose opd should be investigated further in a larger sample. the nfas results were then compared to the mbss results to determine validity. criterion validity criterion validity determined the extent to which the nfas agreed with the gold standard (mbss) measuring the same variable. measures to determine criterion validity included the predictive ability, sensitivity, specificity and accuracy of the instrument. the comparative results are presented in table 4. table 4: comparison between the mbss and nfas results (n = 20). sensitivity and specificity when comparing the mbss and nfas outcomes in table 4, all six neonates who presented with opd were correctly identified with the nfas; however, three were incorrectly identified, resulting in a false positive rate of 21.4%. this comparison revealed the nfas presented with a sensitivity of 100% when identifying opd in neonates. the specificity of 78.6% reflects the probability of the nfas to determine that a neonate does not present with dysphagia. predictive diagnostic ability of the neonatal feeding assessment scale the ppv and npv were calculated using the data in table 4. the ppv was 100% (6 / 6 × 100) and the npv was 78.6% (11 / 14 × 100). the higher the ppv and npv (closer to 100%), the better the new assessment scale is at diagnosing opd when compared to the gold standard (parikh, mathai, parikh, chandra sekhar & thomas, 2008). based on the ppv and npv scores the nfas showed adequate predictive ability to determine when opd would be present or absent. it was concluded that among those participants who had opd the predictive ability of dysphagia being present was 100% and among those participants who did not have opd the predictive ability of not having dysphagia was 78.6%. diagnostic accuracy of neonatal feeding assessment scale compared to modified barium swallow studies the overall accuracy was calculated using the specificity and sensitivity data. the accuracy of agreement on diagnosis of opd between the nfas and mbss was 85% (11 + 6/20 × 100). the closer the accuracy score is to 100%, the better agreement there is between the newly developed instrument and the gold standard (dawson & trapp, 2004). the nfas therefore presented with good preliminary sensitivity (100%) (dawson & trapp, 2004). specificity was also considered to be good (dawson & trapp, 2004) at 78.6%. an assessment tool with a high specificity, sensitivity, ppv, npv and accuracy is considered valuable in clinical practice (lalkhen & mccluskey, 2008). the participants not diagnosed with opd on mbss presented with oesophageal dysphagia or normal swallowing ability. apart from the six participants diagnosed with opd on the nfas and the mbss, the mbss revealed additional results as expected. based on mbss results, 40% (n= 8) of the participants presented with oesophageal dysphagia, 10% (n = 2) had opd co-occurring with oesophageal dysphagia and four participants had normal swallowing. two of the six neonates diagnosed with opd on the nfas presented with this co-occurrence. inter-rater reliability inter-rater reliability for all the sections and diagnostic outcome of the nfas between two independent raters were determined using half of the sample (n = 10). a kappa value of greater than 0.410 was considered a minimal reliability criterion and a p-bar value of 0.50 (dawson & trapp, 2004). the inter-rater reliability calculations of each section of the instrument are presented in table 5. table 5: inter-rater reliability of sub-sections and diagnostic outcome of the nfas (n = 10). the inter-rater reliability for two of the five sections of the instrument demonstrated substantial agreement beyond chance. in the combined sections a and b as well as for section d, the assessment criteria were clear (0.90–1.00 p-bar), therefore rendering the kappa calculation obsolete for these sections. for section c the results indicated only slight agreement, which may be due to the variability of infant state during the feeding process. thus the variability inherent to infant state may have increased the difficulty to evaluate this section objectively. the two raters agreed on the instrument outcome in 90% (n = 9) of the cases. the agreement on diagnostic outcome between the two raters was considered substantial beyond chance with an ase of 0.241 (dawson & trapp, 2004). discussion the preliminary performance of the nfas indicated that it is a valid method of assessing neonatal feeding skills, guiding clinicians to diagnose opd and thereby potentially facilitating early detection and management of opd. according to demauro and colleagues, dysphagia is a significant disorder in preterm infants in developing countries, and valid assessment instruments can compensate for the lack of population-based studies (demauro, patel, medoff-cooper, posenscheg & abbasi, 2011). the prevalence of opd (45%) found in this sample was higher than in some other studies (demauro et al., 2011). in 2014, zehetgruber and colleagues reported a prevalence range of dysphagia in their sample of preterm and lbw infants of 25%–35% (zehetgruber et al., 2014). the higher prevalence rate in this study may not be accurate because prevalence cannot be determined on such a small sample as utilised in this study. the nfas provides more descriptive information on feeding skills, such as detailed information on stress cues and infant state, than the mbss. therefore it may also offer more intervention guidelines to inexperienced clinicians. criterion validity the high sensitivity and specificity of the nfas provide evidence of the ability of the scale to accurately diagnose the presence of opd and in turn to also recognise the absence of opd, rendering very few false positives. there appears to be limited information on the sensitivity and specificity properties of comparable assessments for oral motor difficulties in neonates and infants, such as the efs, nomas and soma (da costa, van den engel-hoek & bos, 2008). the diagnostic accuracy (85%) of the nfas and its good predictive ability (dawson & trapp, 2004) in clinical use showed that the scale is capable of measuring what it intends to measure. as expected of a direct instrumental observational procedure, the mbss gave additional diagnoses. different types of dysphagia exist in neonates, depending on the stage of swallowing that is affected (pados et al., 2016). different types of dysphagia can also co-occur. the mbss diagnosed oesophageal dysphagia and clearly showed the co-occurrence of the two types of dysphagia, opd and oesophageal dysphagia. because the focus of slts is on assessment and intervention of opd, preliminary results indicate that the nfas could serve this purpose. all participants who truly presented with opd were identified. when relying on clinical assessments only in contexts where mbss is not available, the three false positive opd results may not be viewed as disadvantageous. further research is required to determine whether subsequent assessments on the same neonate using the nfas may show different results. inter-rater reliability the preliminary testing of the nfas showed that acceptable inter-rater reliability was present. due to the substantial agreement beyond chance achieved in the inter-rater reliability results, it appears that more than one clinician is likely to obtain the same results when using the nfas. the pre-assessment training and test administration guidelines may be sufficient to support a clinician to obtain consistent results when administering the scale. the nfas compares favourably with other widely used instruments investigating components of feeding skills, such as the nomas (palmer et al., 1993) and the soma (reilly et al., 2000), which presented with good inter-rater reliability for clinical use in neonates and infants older than 8 months, respectively. a 2008 study by da costa and van der schans determined the inter-rater reliability of the nomas ranged from moderate to substantial agreement (kappa: 0.40–0.65) (da costa & van der schans, 2008), although palmer et al. (1993), the developers of the scale, did not test the final scale for reliability. the soma presented with a kappa of <0.75 on a sample of 10 infants, indicating excellent agreement beyond chance (reilly, skuse, mathisen & wolke, 1995). the authors of the efs (pados et al., 2016) stated that intraand inter-rater reliability had been found to be stable and acceptable, but no data were provided to support this statement (da costa & van der schans, 2008). the preliminary results thus indicate good reliability (dawson & trapp, 2004) of the nfas. scoring criteria the weighting of the different sections of the nfas, in contributing to the diagnosis of opd, could not be determined adequately in this study due to the small sample size. it appears that state observation (section b) may be difficult to score due to the fleeting nature of infant states and fluidity between some state changes during a feeding session. simultaneous observation of different feeding skills in the infant is required when using the nfas. while focusing on the oral area to observe aspects such as non-nutritive sucking (nns) and the neonate’s behavioural response to nns during feeding, there may also be subtle stress cues and state changes taking place, with the result that some of the state changes and stress cues may be missed. in premature and lbw infants, state is influenced by a variety of factors, such as energy expenditure and endurance during feeding (arvedson & brodsky, 2002; thoyre, park, pados & hubbard, 2013). nugent, keefer, minear, johnson and blanchard (2007) concurred that the accuracy of state observation requires that clinicians gain clinical experience and attend continued professional development training opportunities in the observation and interpretation of neonatal and infant behaviour. because state regulation not only impacts on feeding but on the full spectrum of infant behaviour, it may not directly contribute to the diagnostic process during feeding assessment. observation of state regulation is, however, recognised in the literature and other studies (browne & ross, 2011; nugent et al., 2007). evaluation of state regulation may help the clinician to support the parent to identify infant states and understand that certain activities are more appropriate while the infant is in one particular state than another. for example, feeding is best supported when an infant is in one of the alert states (stage 4, quiet alert) without showing distress (browne & ross, 2011; nugent et al., 2007). feeding in itself also acts as the initial primary regulator of physiological state, as the very young infant uses primitive brainstem–visceral circuits during feeding as the underlying mechanism for state regulation (browne & ross, 2011). conclusion in summary, neonatal dysphagia will remain a complex problem that requires multidisciplinary, multidimensional assessment and treatment. in order to increase effective management of neonatal feeding and swallowing difficulties, the standard of clinical assessment should improve in developing countries where services are not well regulated. the use of validated neonatal feeding assessment instruments should take priority to support evidence-based practice (miller, 2009; pados et al., 2016). a comprehensive clinical assessment instrument addressing the overall feeding process in neonates that also provides systematic guidance in clinical decision-making for the diagnosis of opd is recommended. the nfas highlights the subtleties of the feeding process and describes procedures of observation and elicitation that should not be overlooked during clinical assessment. multidisciplinary team members and newly qualified or inexperienced clinicians should be able to use such an instrument if sufficiently prompted by the systematic procedures for administration outlined in the tool. the different sections and items in the nfas may assist to describe the feeding profile of high-risk neonates and consequently enable early and accurate clinical diagnosis of opd in the absence of available instrumental assessments in resource-constrained contexts. the validity of an assessment instrument is its real capacity to measure what it proposes to measure. this preliminary attempt at validation of the nfas was performed by comparing it to the mbss. a larger sample will be utilised to determine psychometric properties of the nfas for clinical use in a follow-up study. in addition, the contribution of the different sections of the nfas to the eventual diagnosis of opd in a neonate will also be investigated. acknowledgements competing interests the authors declare that they have no financial or personal relationships that may have inappropriately influenced them in writing this article. authors’ contributions m.v. was the project leader, collected and analysed data, and was responsible for preparing the manuscript. a.k. was also responsible for preparing the manuscript. b.v. only made initial contributions to research design. m.g. provided statistical support throughout the project. references american speech-language-hearing association (asha). (2008). roles and responsibilities of speech-language pathologists in early intervention: technical report. retrieved june 05, 2015, from www.asha.com arvedson, j.c., & brodsky, l. (2002). pediatric swallowing and feeding: assessment and management. clifton park, ny: thomson delmar learning. botha, l., & schoeman, j. (2011). a national survey of the clinical assessment practices of speech-language therapists regarding paediatric dysphagia. unpublished honours dissertation, university of pretoria, pretoria. browne, j.v., & ross, e.s. (2011). eating as a neurodevelopmental process for high-risk newborns. clinical perinatology, 38, 731–743. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.clp.2011.08.004 da costa, s.p., van den engel-hoek, l., & bos, a.f. (2008). sucking and swallowing in infants and diagnostic tools. journal of perinatology, 28, 247–257. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/sj.jp.7211924 da costa, s.p., & van der schans, c. (2008). the reliability of the neonatal oral-motor assessment scale. acta paediatrica, 97, 21–26. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/j.1651-2227.2007.00577.x dawson, b., & trapp, r.g. (2004). basic and clinical biostatistics. (4th edn.). new york: lange medical books. demauro, s.b., patel, p.r., medoff-cooper, b., posencheg, m., & abbasi, s. (2011). postdischarge feeding patterns in earlyand late-preterm infants. clinical pediatrics, 50(10), 957–962. http://dx.doi.org/10.1177/0009922811409028 ensher, g.l., & clark, d.a. (2009). the interface of physiology and medical environments. in g.l. ensher, d.a. clark & n.s. songer (eds.), families, infants, and young children at risk: pathways to best practice (pp. 211–222). baltimore, md: paul h. brookes publishing co. gooding, j.s., cooper, l.g., blaine, a.i., franck, l.s., howse, j.l., & berns, s.d. (2011). family support and family-centred care in the nicu: origins, advances, impact. seminars in perinatology, 35(1), 20–28. http://dx.doi.org/10.1053/j.semperi.2010.10.004 hall, k.d. (2001). pediatric dysphagia: resource guide. san diego, ca: singular thomson learning. hernanz-schulman, m., goske, m.j., bercha, i.h., & strauss, k.j. (2011). pause and pulse: then steps that help manage radiation dose during pediatric fluoroscopy. pediatric imaging, 197, 475–481. http://dx.doi.org/10.2214/ajr.10.6122 hiorns, m.p., & ryan, m.m. (2006). current practices in paediatric videofluorsocopy. pediatric radiology, 36(9), 911–919. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/s00247-006-0124-3 lalkhen, a.g., & mccluskey, a. (2008). clinical tests: sensitivity and specificity. continuing education in anaesthesia, critical care & pain, 8(6), 194–199. landis, j.r., & koch, g.g. (1977). the measurement of observer agreement for categorical data. biometrics, 33, 159–174. http://dx.doi.org/10.2307/2529310 meline, t. (2010). a research primer for communication sciences and disorders. boston, ma: allyn & bacon. miller, c.k. (2009). updates on pediatric feeding and swallowing. current opinion in otolaryngology & head and neck surgery, 17(3), 194–199. nugent, j.k., keefer, c.h., minear, s., johnson, l.c., & blanchard, y. (2007). understanding newborn behaviour and early relationships: the newborn behavioural observations (nbo) systems handbook. baltimore, md: paul h. brookes publishing company. pados, b.f., park, j., estrem, h., & awotwi, a. (2016). assessment tools for evaluation of oral feeding in infants younger than 6 months. advances in neonatal care, 16(2), 143–150. http://dx.doi.org/10.1097/anc.0000000000000255 palmer, m.m., crawley, k., & blanco, i. (1993). neonatal oral-motor assessment scale: a reliability study. journal of perinatology, 13, 28–35. parikh, r., mathai, a., parikh, s., chandra sekhar, g., & thomas, r. (2008). understanding and using sensitivity, specificity and predictive values. indian journal of opthalmology, 56(1), 45–50. http://dx.doi.org/10.4103/0301-4738.37595 pressman, h. (2010). dysphagia and related assessment and management in children with hiv/aids. in d. swanepoel & b. louw (eds.). hiv/aids: related communication, hearing and swallowing disorders (pp. 351–384). oxfordshire: plural publishing. reilly, s., skuse, d., mathisen, b., & wolke, d. (1995). the objective rating of oral-motor functions during feeding. dysphagia, 10, 177–191. http://dx.doi.org/10.1007/bf00260975 reilly, s., skuse, d., & wolke, d. (2000). soma: schedule for oral motor assessment. london: whurr publishers. rogers, b., & arvedson, j. (2005). assessment of infant oral sensorimotor and swallowing function. mental retardation and developmental disabilities research reviews, 11, 74–82. http://dx.doi.org/10.1002/mrdd.20055 scott, m.v., fujii, a.m., behrman, r.h., & dillon, j.e. (2014). diagnostic ionizing radiation exposure in premature infants. journal of perinatology, 34(5), 392–395. http://dx.doi.org/10.1038/jp.2013.141 south african speech language hearing association (sashla). (2011b). guidelines: early communication intervention. retrieved july 13, 2015, from www.saslha.co.za south african speech language hearing association (sashla). (2011a). guidelines: paediatric dysphagia. retrieved july 18, 2011, from www.saslha.co.za swigert, n.b. (2010). the source for pediatric dysphagia. east moline, il: linguisystems. thoyre, s.m., park, j., pados, b., & hubbard, c. (2013). developing a co-regulated, cue-based feeding practice: the critical role of assessment and reflection. journal of neonatal nursing, 19, 139–148. http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jnn.2013.01.002 thoyre, s.m., shaker, c.s., & pridham, k.f. (2005). the early feeding skills assessment for preterm infants. neonatal network, 24(3), 7–16. http://dx.doi.org/10.1891/0730-0832.24.3.7 viviers, m.m., kritzinger, a.m., & vinck, b. (2016). development of a clinical feeding assessment scale for neonates in south africa. south african journal of communication disorders, 63(1), a148. http://dx.doi.org/10.4102/sajcd.v63i1.148 zehetgruber, n., boedeker, r.h., kurth, r., faas, d., zimmer, k.p., & heckmann, m. (2014). eating problems in very low birthweight children are highest during the first year and independent risk factors include duration of invasive ventilation. acta paediatrica, 10, e424–e438. http://dx.doi.org/10.1111/apa.12730 appendix 1 table 1-a1: data collection procedures for the nfas. page twelve journal of the south african logopedic society the treatment of habitual dysphonia joan h. van thai, m.b.e., f.c.s.t. from time to time patients consult a laryngologist complaining of hoarseness and loss of voice for which no cause can be found either in the larynx or its nerve supply. indirect, laryngoscopy commonly shows a deficiency in the approximation of the vocal folds, occasionally interference by the ventricular bands during phonation and various other laryngeal dysfunctions. by "loss of voice" they may mean a variety of things, but hardly ever real aphonia; loss of carrying power of voice, restriction of range of pitch, changes in its quality such as huskiness, cracking or harshness all enter into the picture, which is too varied to be described in all its kaleidoscopic manifestations. the patients in question are either professional voice users (in which category one should include amateur performers, and people who do a lot of talking in the course of their work, such as interviewing, telephoning etc.) who find their voices no longer adequate to meet the demands made on them, or people who have become justifiably alarmed about their chronic hoarseness and wisely consult a laryngologist, and in both instances are reassured about their condition, told it is due to habitual misuse of the voice and referred to the speech therapist. much now depends on the importance which the patient attaches to the quality of the voice whether they make use of the services of the speech therapist, available to them free of charge under the national health service, so that the question of expense does not enter into it. the decision may depend on common sense, but it may also be influenced by a psychological overlay of the dysphonia; thus refusal may be due to difficulty in getting time off from work for someone not a professional voice user, or to an unconscious desire to retain the symptom which is of some value to the patient whose dysphonia is of a psychogenic nature. similarly agreement to have treatment may be on a rational basis, the necessity to recover the voice for professional or social reasons, or it may be for the sake of being interesting and satisfying a subconscious urge for attention. those who refuse voice therapy are of no further interest; but a careful assessment of the reason for the vocal dysfunction must be made in those who seek rehabilitation. the situation may be the uncomplicated one of a teacher, public speaker or the like on whose voice special demands have been made without their having been trained to meet these requirements; it may be a singer who has been badly taught, or who has misunderstood his instructor's directions and explanations. these people are in a category where corrective work is required, and. this work is on the borders of normal voice training and "voice therapy": a routine as set out below can be followed. psychogenic dysfunctions can approximate' closely to hysterical aphonia — an unconscious desire to shake off responsibilities, e.g. a headmaster who had, suffered from laryngitis and been unable for that reason to address the end of term assembly began to have recur-1 ring bouts of loss of voice — he carried on with his work with enthusiasm except if addressing the assembled school. (given insight into' his recurring dysphonia he recovered completely with a minimum of direct voice training). in many instances however there is no hysterical element in the voice change, but it is expressive of a state of mind of the patient. normally the voice expresses our feeling; thus in "people in quandaries" of a persistent nature the harsh and cracked, feeble and husky, low constricted or high pitched strident sound reflects their emotional condition, e.g. a harassed housewife who had suffered much grief and worry over a period of two or three years, torn between her duty to a widowed mother and to her husband, her whole posture and facial expression as well as her feeble, croaking voice portrayed her mental stress; indirect laryngoscopy showed that the right vocal fold made a spasmodic abductor movement posteriorly during phonation, though the anterior part and the l.v.c. adducted normally. considerably more in the nature of reassurance and relief from tension by relaxation and freely talking about her troubles was done concurrently with vocal retraining than would be envisaged for cases of vocal dysfunction only due to ignorance of good technique. it would be equally wrong to assume that there must be psychological complications in these cases as it would be to take the opposite view. thus, as in all cases referred for speech therapy, a full case history has to be compiled. it is wise, however much of ones time one devotes to this at the first interview, to take some of the preliminary steps in vocal retraining from the start, since these people usually expect that something be done. to affront the patient at this early stage by too obvious delving would have a very serious affect on rapport, particularly if there are no psychological complications. no harm can be done by doing postural exercises, even if soon after the course of treatment has begun one realizes one is dealing with a psycho-neurosis beyond the scope of the speech therapist, in· that case one should hand over to the psycho-therapist and voice therapy is contra-indicated. in the less serious psycho-^ genie cases a combination of counselling and direct voice correction can safely be undertaken. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) journal of the south african logopedic society page thirteen the routine of voice correction must include: 1. postural exercises. 2. simple breathing exercises. 3. exercises in projection of the voice. 4. establishing of good habits of voice production in speaking and reading; for singers — also in song. postural exercises it is the posture of the head and neck that is of supreme importance; unless the patient has to stand and speak, and must therefore acquire a good stance, it simplifies matters to do these exercises seated. a mirror allowing him to see head and should is essential. there is invariably misplaced effort, even though where there is failure in adduction of the vocal folds the first impression is the opposite; the deficient adduction may be due to activity of the abductors, contrary to the law of reciprocal innervation, when these should relax. let the patient be seated on a chair that offers good support to the thighs and the back, without tilting the head forward or back.· 1. ensure that the chin is held at the correct angle; commonly it is tilted slightly upwards, sometimes it is pushed forward as well; occasionally, but not very often, the chin is tucked in. stand behind the patient, take his head between your hands and firmly but gently move it from side to side, down and up in a forward direction, and finish with the head held straight, chin on a horizontal plane. let the patient bring the back of his fingers, held straight, under the chin to feel the position; care must be taken he does not tilt his hand up to meet the chin if he happens to be holding the chin thus. gradually he must learn to do without looking in the mirror or feeling the position of the head with his hand, and become aware whether he is holding his head straight wtihout strain, witout such aids. 2. let the patient bend the head forward and straighten it again, | several times running. this to be done without jerks, j and at a moderate pace. 3. negative practice—bend head forward; straighten; crane a little; straighten again. the craning phase must be much shorter than each of the others. 4. jaw exercises — patient places a hand on either cheek and gently strokes downwards bringing the mandible down and so opening the mouth about an inch. close again and repeat several times. open and close jaw effortlessly without stroking. (chewing exercise as recommended by e. froeschels should also be done.) breathing exercises no elaborate breathing exercises need be done, except possibly by singers and those who have to speak in large auditoria. there is every likelihood, however, that these patients have some unpropitious habits, such as raising the shoulders, over-breathing or keeping the thorax rigid; nearly all will exhale with a fricative sound (not a consonant) which indicates constriction of the pharynx or oral cavity, when they breathe out through the open mouth. n.b.—be sure that they open the mouth in the manner practised under jaw exercises, neither too little nor too much; do not allow them to close the mouth till the end of the expiratory phase, unless practising breathing out with formation of speech sounds, such as m. 1. place hands on sides, just below the sternum, with finger tips meeting. no pressure to be exercised by the hands. breathe out through nose as a preliminary, fingers will overlap a little. now breathe in to a count of two, out for two; both through the nose. the hands are there to feel the lateral movement; watch for shoulder heaving, swaying of the torso and other useless extraneous movements. 2. breathe in throygh the nose, out through the mouth, silently. take the exercises at the normal respiratory rate of about 17 to the minute. 3. slightly prolong the expiratory phase, but leave the inspiratory as before. 4. while breathing out change the position of lips and tongue, as if you were about to whisper "a-u" three or four times running; vary the exercise with "a-i" but still make no sound. 5. allow the breath to "pour out" audibly without friction, for the same vowel series. "ha-hu, ha-hi"* (n.b.—do not use the word whisper to the patient, for he will certainly start to use a forced whisper if you do. do not encourage audible exhalation with the articulatory organs static, this is more likely to lead to strain, and these exercises are preparatory to training in voice projection, which is similarly better done on a series of syllables than on a single vowel.) *a random pattern of vowels may prove satisfactory taken with the chewing technique. humiming 1. find a note comfortably about the middle of the patient's range. let him hum "mmm" softly for two seconds or so, placing a finger on the lips the while to feel the vibrations reaching them. 2. prolong the humming a little growing louder and r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) page fourteen journal of the south african logopedic society then allowing the sound to diminish again. pluck the lip while doing so, and a "twanging" sound should be heard if the voice is well projected, with full nasal resonance. it is possible to miss the twanging in spite of satisfactory resonance if " n " is being sung instead of "m", however.) in the case of singers who have learned a wrong technique it is better to practise on speaking glides at first instead of on a sung note. similarly there are people who believe they are unable to sing and demur at sung exercises, for whom speaking glides only should be used. 3. either hum on a series of singing notes, or on a spoken glide on "m". for singers gradually increase the range of the notes up and down the scale. other voice projection exercises. 4.. open mouth, as practised before, and sigh out gently on a descending glide. no hard attack (glottal stop) no scraping. : gradually reduce the sighing element and emit " a " or another vowel on a descending or ascending glide without constriction in any part of the vocal apparatus. 5. using " m " as initial, practise a series of syllables with the vowels practised during breathing exercises, "ma-mu, ma-mu, ma-mu" "ma-mi, ma-mi, ma-mi" "ma-mu-ma-mi, ma-mu-ma-mi". 6. proceed to words with similar sound patterns e.g. english dutch may moon mijn moe my man mijn man sentences my mother made some de molenaar maalt nie marmalade/ one morning/ meer/ voo r maandag/ last month. maar mijn meel is op. the common error of constricting the voice at the end of a sentence can be overcome by the following simple device — pretend you are going to say a word or two more but donot say them aloud. in the above practice sentences the patient is told the words up "to the second stroke, but instructed only to speak those up to the first stroke "marmalade" or "meer" aloud, and say the rest "in his head". next tell them the remainder of the sentence, allow them to speak aloud to "morning" or maandag", but say the rest in their heads. finally allow them to speak the whole sentence, but still keep up the pretence that there is a word or two to follow. 7. practice on words of little importance. induce the patient to transfer his technique, so far depending on the propitious initial m, to other words and phrases. in order to avoid any strain that may be set up by an earnest attempt to remember the words of •the. exercise, it is best at this point to use such devices as counting, repeating the names of the days and months, familiar telephone numbers, conventional phrases of greeting etc. the device "think another word'is to come" can again be used, e.g. count to ten but imagine you are going on to eleven; say "good morning" pretend you were about to say "good morning, sir" and change your mind about "sir". reading aloud. this is a useful medium for establishing correct habits, but it has many pitfalls. for those who are not accustomed to reading aloud it is unpropitious, since it causes stress militating against the use of their new technique, and if they never will have to readaloud in their daily life it is purposeless to practise the art. for those who are accustomed to read aloud there is the pitfall of an acquired special reading voice, so that such .practice causes relapse, thus it should be postponed till the last stages of rehabilitation of voice. speeches. these can be more safely introduced at the intermediate stage. the subject matter will depend on the interests of the speaker — ranging from the retelling a film recently seen to an expert snort lecture on biochemistry. material to learn by heart. particularly where reading aloud is to be avoided and where there is little originality of thought for speeches, it is well to provide practice sentences of all kinds, and rhymes to suit the individual, but always short ones so that there is no effort to remember them. when the patients have reached a point where they are quite sure of using the right technique and have shaken off wrong •habits, allow them to go to teachers of speech or song if they wish tp improve further or to learn to address audiences. the speech therapist must emphasize that 1 shouting does not make for audibility, but correct projection does and clear articulation is needed for intelligibility. r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) 82 sajcd • vol 57 • december 2010 cpd december 2010 1. low health literacy/recall skills and intercultural differences may affect clients regardless of their: a) socio-economic status b) age c) race d) all of the above. 2. almost all the interventions that aim to maximise health literacy and ability to recall clinical information: a) are too expensive and time consuming to implement b) are complex and need to be designed to meet each individual client’s needs c) are too modern and won’t be easily accepted by clients d) are only for use by clients who are technology-orientated. 3. true (a) or false (b) – click on the correct answer: health literacy refers to the extent to which individuals can effectively obtain, process, and understand health information in order to make appropriate decisions about their (or their child’s) health. 4. assessment of children’s speech in south africa: a) is a challenge because of a lack of resources in all languages b) should always occur in english and afrikaans c) should always occur in the speech-language pathologist (slp)’s first language d) is something that most slps feel very confident about. 5. parent involvement: a) is seldom used by slps working with children’s speech in south africa b) is optional in bowen and cupples’ (2006) pact programme c) is best encouraged through home programmes d) was used by most slps in the survey carried out in the western cape, as well as in a british study by joffe and pring (2008). 6. the most desirable emergent literacy areas to assess are: a) alphabet knowledge and concepts about print b) those that contribute to and are predictive of later reading and writing, and are amenable to change through intervention c) representations of the orthography of written language and name writing – invented spelling d) oral language, including syntactic, lexical and narrative abilities. 7. true (a) or false (b) – click on the correct answer: the combination of limited proficiency in the language of education, minority status and a low socio-economic status is reported to be a strong predictor that a child will fail to learn to read and write well. 8) self-identities are based on: a) other people’s reactions to individuals b) the meaning attributed to individual characteristics c) individuals’ understanding of themselves d) early life experiences . 9. self-identities: a) occur in isolation only b) may co-exist in varying relationships c) remain static throughout an individual’s life d) are always in conflict. 10. roeper (2004) states that teachers should attempt to remove the ambiguities for english additional language learners by establishing linguistic contexts that support and make these skills contextually clear. measures that teachers may use to assist learners include: a) repetition, explanation, giving examples, visual supports b) drill work c) reinforcement d) recitation. 11. language skills that develop during the foundation phase are important for academic development and teachers can therefore play a role in the effective learning of academic language. teachers in south africa: a) are trained to develop language learning skills b) are too busy to work on language learning during classroom teaching c) may be unaware of this responsibility as well as lack the necessary training d) can access speech-language therapists to assist in classroom language intervention. 12. knowledge acquisition at an ‘awareness level’: a) is the lowest level of knowledge acquisition b) can change practices c) improves outcomes d) is easily translated into practical situations. 13. teacher confidence is directly related to teacher competence and their ability to facilitate learning. a) high levels of confidence may negatively impact on teaching b) high levels of confidence are a positive attribute and may benefit learners c) high levels of confidence indicate a lack of knowledge d) high levels of confidence indicate a lack of skill. cpd questionnaires must be completed online via www.cpdjournals.org.za. after submission you can check the answers and print your certificate. the south african journal of communication disorders sajcd vol 57 • december 2010 • sajcd 83 the south african journal of communication disorders cpd 14. children with fetal alcohol spectrum disorder (fasd): a) have many areas of weakness related to alcohol exposure b) display negative sequelae of prenatal alcohol exposure, but may also have positive characteristics c) have no protective factors d) have a significant risk profile. 15. children with fasd demonstrate: a) normal language development with the right stimulation b) deviant language development c) different language development depending on alcohol exposure d) language impairment, communication delay, verbosity and possible processing disorders. 16. aural rehabilitation services in developing countries receive low health care priority: a) because there are less people with hearing loss in developing countries b) due to competing health care demands from diseases with high mortality rates such as hiv/aids c) because training in aural rehabilitation is a problem d) due to poor awareness among professionals. 17. a south african sample of hearing aid users reported: a) no benefit from hearing aids in the majority of cases b) benefit only with binaural hearing aids c) general satisfaction and benefit from hearing aids d) no limitations to participation after using hearing aids. 18. hearing impairment: a) affects quality of life, as it may have an adverse effect on physical, cognitive, emotional, behavioural and social functioning b) is an exclusive feature of an individual c) affects individuals regardless of the environment d) requires audiological rehabilitation only. 19. professional voice users include individuals who are directly dependent on vocal communication for their livelihood. these individuals include: a) factory workers and teachers b) singers, actors, teachers and clergy c) doctors, accountants and lawyers d) labourers, domestic workers and office assistants 20. teachers are at risk for developing voice disorders. factors that affect teachers’ vocal hygiene include: a) the fact that the majority of teachers are female b) stress related to the teaching environment c) poor awareness of voice problems d) large numbers of students and classroom design requiring teachers to use a loud voice for extended periods and in potentially dusty environments. tongue troubles • ϊς s.b. v, renen. my first experience of tongue troubles begins during a reading lesson when i was brought out to the front of my class at a junior school and asked whether i was trying to annoy ray teacher or attract attention by reading in such a hesitant and tense manner. when i was unable to give a satisfactory reply it was taken for granted that i was just being awkward and i was beaten before the whole class and ordered to stop "stuttering". prom that point onwards i took the-same path as hundrede of perfectly normal "stutterers" had taken before me and, however much i regret it, others will continue to suffer unless an attempt is made to change the attitudes and policies, the evaluations, of parents and teachers concerning the child as a person and speaker. prom that moment onwarde i apparently ceased to be a "normal" child to so-called "normal" people, and i was looked upon as somebody quite different. in fact i was looked upon as a stutterer. at high school teachers soon realised that in standard vii there was a stutterer, who thue had to be handled gently. i wae never to be asked to read aloud or recite in fact my stutter was to be made as obvioue as possible to all the others in the class. fortunately for me i was. able to hold my own amongst the boys as i excelled at sport. if i could not speak normally to my superiors (i never stuttered otherwise) i could at least run faster, or tackle better than anyone else my age, which gave me some sense of satisfaction and c onfi dene e. everything went on quite normally until i left school. i was a stutterer well enough by now everybody knew it in spite 5 ^em jpretending that they did not. then something happened which brought my "stutter" horribly to the forefront, bince myj childhood days i always had the ambition of one day becoming a soldier. i had been at school during the war but by now i had annexed a matriculation certificate, two athletic: records and a fine testimonial, i accordingly applied for admission to an officers' cadet course. my application was accepted and i was called before a selection board. i remember entering the room feeling confident and at ease but all of a sudden i found myself face to race with a major general. he asked me a question and i , opened my mouth to answer, but i was concentrating more on the thought "don't stutter now or show him that you are at stutterer" than the reply to his question. the result r ep ro du ce d by s ab in et g at ew ay u nd er li ce nc e gr an te d by th e p ub lis he r (d at ed 2 01 2) . rejected as unsuitable for training as an officer. here i was brought face to face with the truth i was a stutterer. could anything "be done about it? i racked my brain, i read books;· in fact, i even thought of doing away with myself. then i was referred to a speech therapist. it was not long before i was walking into shops and asking the prices of different articles definitely faking as i asked, though it required some courage in the first instance. i was beginning to face up to my problem more objectively as i stopped trying to hide the fact that i stuttered from others. slowly it dawned on me that if i walked up to a person and stuttered yes, stuttered as if it· was the most common thing in the world he would not notice that there was anything particularly defective about my speech, and would therefore not react differently towards me. to prove to myself that it was the way i reacted to people first that mattered i purchased the most "zoot" tie (yellow with pink elephants) and colourful socks, which i wore to a party soon afterwards. as i entered the room i could feel the people looking at the tie but i reacted as if everything was perfectly normal the result nobody even remarked about my tie. as i sat down i made sure to show my socks, and one girl burst out laughing at them. i asked what the joke was, and then she suddenly seemed to come to her senses, as she could not answer me. that evening was certainly a triumph for me, slowly but surely i was gaining confidence. if i could wear that tie without myself feeling conspicuous, it would be accepted as part of me. if i therefore stuttered without feeling ill at ease or self conscious it would be accepted as my manner of speech. i enrolled at a public speaking class next and the first evening everyone had to rise and say why he had come to the class. the first said that they found it necessary for business purposes to be able to talk fluently and easily in public, etc. then came my turn no worrying about stuttering this time i faked for an extra long period on my first word and watched all th